Skip to main content

Full text of "Handbook of physiology; a critical, comprehensive presentation of physiological knowledge and concepts"

See other formats


a ; 
a ' 


a  I 
<-n ! 
133  i 
ru  i 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


SECTION  1:  Neurophysiology,  volume  i 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 

A  critical,  comprehensive  presentation 
of  physiological  knowledge  and  concepts 


SECTION   1: 


Neurophysiology 


VOLUME  I 


Editor-in-Chief:    JOHN    FIELD 
Section  Editor:    H.     W.    MAGOUN 
Executive  Editor:    VICTOR    E.    HALL 


American  Physiological  Society,  Washington,  d.  c,  1959 


@  Copyrighl  ig5<),  American  Physiological  Society 

Library  of  Congress  Catalog  Card  No.  ^g-isg^y 

Printed  m  the  United  States  of  America  by  Waverly  Press,  Inc.,  Baltimore  2,  .Maryland 

Distributed  by  Williams  &  M'ilkins  Co.,  Baltimore  2,  Maryland 


Foreword 


The  original  literature  in  the  field  of  physiology 
has  become  so  vast  and  is  growing  so  rapidlv  that 
the  retrieval,  correlation  and  evaluation  of  knowledge 
has  become  with  each  passing  year  a  more  complex 
and  pressing  problem.  Compounding  the  difficulties 
has  been  the  inevitable  trend  toward  fragmentation 
into  smaller  and  smaller  compartments,  both  of 
knowledge  and  of  research  skills.  This  trend  is  not 
only  inevitable,  but  it  is  necessary  to  healthy  growth. 
It  must,  however,  be  accompanied  by  the  develop- 
ment of  mechanisms  for  convenient  and  reliable  re- 
integration in  order  that  knowledge  shall  not  be  lost 
and  research  efTort  wasted. 


The  American  Physiological  Society  has  enlisted 
the  cooperation  of  physiological  scientists  over  the 
world  in  attempting  to  provide  a  mechanism  in  this 
Handbook  of  Physiology  series  for  providing  a  com- 
prehensive but  critical  presentation  of  the  state 
of  knowledge  in  the  various  fields  of  functional 
biology.  It  is  intended  to  cover  the  physiological 
sciences  in  their  entirety  once  in  about  ten  years, 
and  to  repeat  the  process  periodically  thereafter. 

Board  of  Publication  Trustees 
MAURICE    B.    visscHER,    Chairman 

W  I  L  L  I  .^  M     F.     H  .^  M  I  L  T  O  N 
PHILIP     BARD 


Preface 


This  Handbook  of  Physiology ,  like  its  predecessors  from 
von  Haller  on,  is  designed  to  constitute  a  repository 
for  the  body  of  present  pliysioiogical  knowledge, 
systematically  organized  and  presented.  It  is  addressed 
primarily  to  professional  physiologists  and  advanced 
students  in  physiology  and  related  fields.  Its  purpose 
is  to  enable  such  readers,  by  perusal  of  any  Section, 
to  obtain  a  working  grasp  of  the  concepts  of  that  field 
and  of  their  experimental  background  sufficient  for 
initial  planning  of  research  projects  or  preparation 
for  teaching. 

To  accomplish  this  purpose  the  editors  have 
planned  a  book  which  would  differ  from  textbooks  in 
being  more  complete,  more  analytical  and  more 
authoritative.  It  would  differ  from  a  series  of  mono- 
graphs in  being  organized  on  a  consistent  plan  with- 
out important  gaps  between  topics  and  with  as  nearly 
as  possible  the  same  relation  of  intensity  of  coverage 
to  importance  of  topic  throughout.  It  would  differ 
from  publications  emphasizing  new  developments  in 
that  the  background  of  currently  accepted  or  classical 
concepts  would  be  set  forth,  newer  ideas  receiving 
not  more  than  their  due  proportion  of  emphasis 
relative  to  the  whole  body  of  knowledge  in  the  field. 
Finally  it  would  differ  from  a  collection  of  original 
papers  on  a  series  of  topics  in  that  it  would  provide  an 
integrated  condensation  and  evaluation  of  the  mate- 
rial contained  therein.  Moreover,  the  overall  plan 
provides  that  the  key  experimental  findings  in  the 
development  of  each  field  of  investigation  be  de- 
scribed and  discussed  in  sufficient  detail  (with  appro- 
priate illustrations,  quantitati\e  data  and  adequate 
documentation)  to  make  clear  their  nature,  validity 
and  significance  for  the  fundamental  concepts  of  the 
field.  The  success  of  this  endeavor  must  be  left  to  the 
reader's  judgment. 


This  Handbook  stands  as  the  current  representative 
of  an  historic  series  of  efforts  to  collect  and  system- 
atize biological  knowledge — a  series  continued  when 
the  Board  of  Publication  Trustees  of  the  American 
Physiological  Society  decided  in  195;]  to  sponsor  the 
present  undertaking.  A  brief  list  of  notable  prede- 
cessors may  interest  .some  readers.  First  known  of 
the  series  is  a  brief  Sumerian  'pharmacopeia"  dating 
from  perhaps  2100  B.C.  Later  examples  included 
several  Egyptian  papyri  such  as  the  Ebers  and  the 
Edwin  Smith.  Far  more  extensive  compilations  char- 
acterized the  Greco-Roman  period.  Outstanding 
among  those  were  the  Hippocratic  collection  (written 
ijy  several  authors)  and  the  encyclopedic  writings 
associated  with  the  names  of  Aristotle,  Theophrastus, 
Celsus  and  Galen  (Pliny's  work  is  useful  chieflv  to 
the  student  of  folklore).  These  treatises  systematized 
knowledge  of  the  day  over  a  wide  range  and  set  forth 
new  information  based  on  the  authors'  observations. 
Thus  they  combined  the  roles  of  handbook  and  scien- 
tific journal,  a  pattern  that  persisted  until  develop- 
ment of  scientific  journals  (in  the  seventeenth  cen- 
tury). Other  important  compilations  were  made  by 
the  writers  of  the  'Moslem  Renaissance'  such  as  Rhazes 
and  Avicenna,  to  whom  much  of  the  Greco-Roman 
literature  was  available. 

European  biological  compendia  of  the  Christian 
era,  from  the  fourth  century  Physiologus  to  the  exten- 
sive biological  encyclopedias  of  the  sixteenth  and 
seventeenth  centuries,  differed  greatly  in  character 
from  Greco-Roman  and  'Moslem  Renaissance'  work. 
Marked  by  strong  theological  and  anthropocentric 
orientation,  they  lacked  the  descriptive  accuracy  and 
rational  approach  of  the  ancients.  Scientia  was  con- 
sidered ancillary  to  sapientia.  Nature  was  studied 
chiefly    to   obtain    illustrations   for   moral    tales   and 


HANDBOOK   OF   PHVSIOI.OGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


religious  dogmas,  not  to  gain  knowledge  or  insight, 
or  to  learn  how  to  manipulate  and  control  the  en- 
vironment. Writers  showed  little  critical  capacity  and 
failed  to  distinguish  between  the  tfue  and  the  fabu- 
lous, the  important  and  the  trivial.  These  elements 
are  .still  evident  in  such  major  sixteenth  century 
biological  encyclopedias  as  Gesner's  Historiae  Ant- 
malium  (5  volumes,  1551  1587),  and  Aldrovandi's 
Opera  Omnia  (13  volumes,  1399-1677).  In  both  the 
mark  of  the  medieval  Bestiary  is  strong. 

However,  the  tide  was  turning  in  the  sixteenth 
century  despite  these  notable  examples  of  medieval 
Weltanschauung.  The  range  and  precision  of  anatomi- 
cal knowledge  were  greatly  extended  by  publication 
in  1543  of  Vesalius'  De  Hiimani  Corporis  Fabrica.  It  is 
interesting  to  note  that  increasingly  accurate  hand- 
books of  descriptive  botany  began  to  appear.  At  about 
this  time  the  great  transition  from  the  medieval  to 
the  modern  outlook  (the  'scientific  revolution  of  1500- 
1800')  was  under  way.  This  has  been  succinctly 
described  by  Raven:  "Little  by  little,  nonsense  was 
recognized,  fables  were  exploded,  superstitions  were 
unmasked  and  the  world  outlook  built  up  out  of 
these  elements  fell  to  pieces.  The  seemingly  irrelevant 
labors  of  men  like  Turner  or  Penny  to  identify  and 
name  and  describe  bore  fruit  in  a  refusal  to  accept 
tradition  on  authority  and  in  an  insistence  that  state- 
ments must  be  based  upon  observation  and  capable 
of  verification"  (C.  E.  Raven.  English  Naturalists 
from  Neckam  to  Ray.   1947,  p.  227). 

The  rise  of  the  mechanical  philosophy  in  the  seven- 
teenth century  and  the  rationalism  of  the  eighteenth 
furnished  an  intellectual  climate  favorable  for  science. 
This  was  reflected  in  the  papers,  monographs  and 
compendia  produced.  In  the  spirit  of  the  time, 
Diderot,  d'Alembert  and  their  associates  prepared 
the  Encyclopedie  ou  Dictionnaire  Raisonne  Des  Sciences, 
Des  Arts  et  Des  Metiers  (35  volumes,  Paris,  i  751-1 780). 
While  the  major  contribution  of  this  influential  work 
was  to  diffuse  the  rationalist  interpretation  of  the 
universe  in  mechanistic  terms,  it  included  many  con- 
tributions in  the  biological  sciences.  Together  these 
constitute  a  transitional  stage  of  biological  handbook 
— quite  modern  in  spirit  but  not  in  respect  of  fact  or 
concept. 

While  the  Encyclopedic  was  in  preparation  in  Paris, 
the  Swiss  savant  Albrecht  von  Haller  was  compiling 
the  Elementa  Physiologiae  Corporis  Humani  (8  volumes, 
Lausanne,  1 757-1 765).  This  comprised  both  a  hand- 
book of  anatomy  and  physiology  and  a  vehicle  for 
publication  of  much  original  work  by  the  author. 
Compared  to  earlier  work  the  writing  shows  impres- 


sive critical  capacity,  detailed  familiarity  with  tlie 
achievements  of  others,  ability  to  distinguish  the  trivial 
and  the  important  and  over-all  scientific  insight.  This 
was  the  first  of  the  great  series  of  German  Handhuch 
of  physiology. 

The  vast  increase  in  scientific  activity,  with  multi- 
plication of  investigators,  laboratories  and  journals, 
that  characterized  the  nineteenth  century  led  to  more 
frequent  collection  and  systematization  of  knowledge 
in  the  several  active  fields.  This  was  naturally  centered 
in  Germany  where  scientific  activity  was  greatest. 
Notable  examples  of  handbooks  of  physiology  were 
R.  Wagner's  Handworterhuch  der  Physiologic  mit  Ruch- 
sicht  aiif  Physiologisches  Pathologic  (Braunschweig, 
1 842-1 853);  L.  Hermann's  Handhuch  der  Physiulogie 
(Leipzig,  1 879- 1 883);  G.  Richet's  unfinished  Dic- 
tionnaire de  Physiologic  (Paris,  1 895-1 928);  E.  A. 
vSchafer's  Text-Book  of  Physiology  (Edinburgh  and 
London,  1898- 1900);  W.  Nagel's  Handhuch  der 
Physiologic  des  Menschen  (Leipzig,  1905-1910);  the 
massive  Handhuch  der  Normalen  und  Pathologischen 
Physiologic,  mit  Berikksichtigung  der  Experimentellcn 
Pharmakologie,  edited  by  A.  Bethe,  G.  von  Bergmann, 
G.  Embden  and  A.  Ellinger  (Berlin,  1 926-1 932); 
and  our  immediate  predecessor,  G.-H.  Roger  and 
L.  Billet's  Traite  de  Physiologic  Normale  et  Pathologique 
(Paris,  1 933-1 940).  Characteristically  these  hand- 
books comprised  the  contributions  of  many  authors 
and,  in  the  last  two,  collaboration  of  several  editors 
as  well.  These,  with  comparable  coitipilations  in 
cognate  fields  such  as  K.  von  Bardeleben's  Handhuch 
der  Anatomic  des  Menschen  (Jena,  1896-1911)  and 
E.  Abderhalden's  Handhuch  der  Biologischen  Arheits- 
methoden  (Berlin,  1 925-1 939),  have  provided  a  corpus 
of  collected  and  systematized  scientific  knowledge.  A 
notable  feature  of  all  handbooks,  including  the  pres- 
ent one,  is  their  increasingly  international  character, 
reflecting  the  broadening  base  of  the  world  of  science. 

Survey  of  these  codifications  from  the  earliest  on 
provides  a  basis  for  Abraham  Flexner's  trenchant 
comment  on  the  history  of  medicine.  "From  the 
earliest  times  medicine  has  been  a  curious  blend  of 
superstition,  empiricism,  and  that  kind  of  sagacious 
observation  which  is  the  stuff  out  of  which  ultimately 
science  is  made.  Of  these  three  strands — superstition, 
empiricism  and  observation — medicine  was  consti- 
tuted in  the  days  of  the  priest-physicians  of  Egypt  and 
Babylonia;  of  the  same  three  strands  it  is  still  com- 
posed. The  proportions  have,  however,  varied  sig- 
nificantly; an  increasingly  alert  and  determined 
effort,  running  through  the  ages,  has  endeavored  to 
expell  superstition,  to  narrow  the  range  of  empiricism 


PREFACE 


and  to  enlarge,  refine  and  systematize  the  scope  of 
observation.  .  .  .  The  general  trend  of  medicine  has 
been  away  from  magic  and  empiricism  and  in  the 
direction  of  rationality  and  definiteness"  (A.  Flexner. 
Medical  Education.  A  Comparative  Study.  New  York, 
1925).  We  trust  that  continuation  of  this  trend  is 
reflected  in  this  Handbook. 

It  is  difficult  to  acknowledge  properly  the  devoted 
and  effective  work  which  has  made  this  vast  under- 


taking possible.  Its  success  is  due  alike  to  the  con- 
tributors, to  the  editorial  staff  and  to  the  Board  of 
Publication  Trustees  of  the  American  Physiological 
Society.  Alike  to  all  of  these  is  due  the  gratitude  of  the 
world  of  physiologists  for  a  task  well  done. 

JOHN     FIELD 

Editor-in-Chief,  ig§4-ig§8 


Preface  to  the  Section  on  Neurophysiology 


As  the  Editor-in-Chief  has  pointed  out,  tlie  decision 
of  the  American  Physiological  Society  to  sponsor  a 
Handbook  of  Physiology  continues  an  historic  series  of 
efforts  to  collect  and  systematize  knowledge  in  more 
readily  available  forms.  Although  sharing  many  of 
the  features  of  its  predecessors,  the  present  Handbook 
of  Physiology  is  likely  to  be  less  formidable  than  most 
of  them.  Its  goal,  like  that  of  chariot  racing,  has  been 
to  secure  a  balanced  perch  astride  the  rushing  progress 
of  investigative  advance.  It  attempts  to  survey  the 
status  of  physiology  just  past  the  mid-mark  of  the 
twentieth  century.  In  the  case  of  each  topic,  the  com- 
pilative accumulation  of  analytic  data  is  either  intro- 
duced or  concluded  by  synthesizing  comments  of  an 
'elder  statesman'  still  active  in  the  field.  Thus  a  bal- 
ance is  sought  between  the  presentation  of  specific 
information  and  conceptualization  appropriate  to  it. 

Appropriately  also,  the  Handbook  begins  with  con- 
sideration of  the  nervous  system  by  which  the  activities 
of  other  portions  of  the  body  are  coordinated  and 
controlled.  The  nervous  system  remains  the  last  organ 
of  the  body  still  formidably  to  resist  investigative 
attack;  many  fundamental  concepts  of  its  function  lie 
waiting  in  the  future.  Views  proposing  a  spiritual 
basis  for  neural  function  have  obtained  since  classical 
antiquity.  Only  in  the  past  century  have  materialistic 
outlooks  been  effectively  introduced,  first  with  respect 
to  the  nerve  impulse,  then  in  refle.x  function  and,  most 
recently,  in  Russian  views  applying  concepts  of  reflex 
physiolos^y  to  an  understanding  of  higher  activities 
of  the  brain.  In  this  latter  area,  however,  subjective 
experience  and  the  mind  still  receive  major  attention 


in  the  West  from  the  disciplines  of  psychology  and 
psychiatry,  a  testimony  to  continuing  dualistic  points 
of  view  regarding  function  of  the  neural  organ.  In 
contemporary  studies  of  physiological  psychology  the 
gap  between  brain  and  mind  seems  most  rapidly  to 
be  closing;  prominent  representation  of  this  field  is 
probably  the  most  novel  feature  of  the  table  of  con- 
tents of  the  present  Neurophysiology  Section. 

More  than  customarily,  appreciation  should  be 
expressed  to  the  contributing  authors  of  this  Hand- 
hook.  Each  has  been  willing  to  add  to  the  many  energy- 
draining  burdens  of  a  busy  career  the  difficult  task 
of  surveying  a  field  of  investigative  specialty  both  for 
the  benefit  of  associates  and  for  the  general  welfare 
of  physiological  science.  The  remarkably  fine  series 
of  articles  testifies  to  the  generosity  and  skill  of  each 
contributor.  It  is  to  be  hoped  that  reader  appreciation 
may  compensate  these  authors. 

Special  gratitude  should  be  expressed  also  for  the 
efforts  of  the  Executive  Editor,  Victor  Hall.  His  back- 
ground of  editorial  experience  with  the  Annual  Review 
of  Physiology  enabled  the  manifold  labors  of  this 
'sweet-blooded'  man  to  be  performed  so  deftly  as 
perhaps  to  escape  the  attention  of  the  general  reader. 

Hopefully,  all  who  use  this  Handbook  will  wish  as 
I  do  to  thank,  if  only  silently,  the  contributing  authors 
and  the  Executive  Editor  for  their  generous  efforts 
and  to  applaud  them  for  such  a  fine  accomplishment. 


H.   w.   M  A  G  o  u  N 
Section  Editor 


Contents 


VII. 


VIII. 


XIII. 


XV. 


The  historical  development  of 
neurophysiology 

MARY     A.     B.     BRAZIER I 

Neuron  physiology — Introduction 

J.    C.    ECCLES 59 

Conduction  of  the  nerve  impulse 

ICHIJI  TASAKI 75 

Initiation  of  impulses  at  receptors 

J.     A.     B.     GRAY 123 

Synaptic  and  ephaptic  transmission 

HARRY    GRUNDFEST 147 

Skeletal  neuromuscular  transmission 

PAUL    FATT 199 

Autonomic  neuroeffector  transmission 

U.    S.    VON    EULER 2  15 

Neuromuscular  transmission  in 
invertebrates 

E.    J.    FURSHPAN 239 

Brain  potentials  and  rhythms — Introduction 

A.     FESSARD 255 

Identification  and  analysis  of  single  unit 
activity  in  the  central  nervous  system 

KARL    FRANK 261 

Intrinsic  rhythms  of  the  brain 

VV.    GREY    WALTER 279 

The  evoked  potentials 

HSIANG-TUNG    CHANG 299 

Changes  associated  with  forebrain 
excitation  processes:  d.c.  potentials 
of  the  cerebral  cortex 

JAMES    L.    o'lEARY 

SIDNEY    GOLDRING 315 

The  physiopathology  of  epileptic  seizures 

HENRI    GASTAUT 

M.     FISCHER-WILLIAMS 329 

Sensory  mechanisms — Introduction 

lord    E.     D.     ADRIAN 365 


XIX. 


XXI. 


XVI.  Nonphotic  receptors  in  lower  forms 

hansjochem   autrum 369 

XVII.  Touch  and  kinesthesis 
jerzv  e.  rose 

VERNON    B.    MOUNTCASTLE 387 

XVIII.  Thermal  sensations 

YNGVE    ZOTTERM.'^N 43 1 

Pain 

WILLI.\M    H.    SWEET 459 

The  sense  of  taste 

CARL    PFAFFMANN 5O7 

The  sense  of  smell 

W.     R.     ADEY 535 

Vestibular  mechanisms 

B.  E.  GERNANDT 549 

Excitation  of  auditory  receptors 

HALLOWELL    DAVIS 565 

Central  auditory  mechanisms 

HARLOW    W.     ADES 585 

Vision — Introduction 

H.    K.    HARTLINE 615 

Photosensitivity  in  invertebrates 

LORUS  J.    MILNE 

MARGERY    MILNE 62  I 

The  image-forming  mechanism  of  the  eye 

GLENN    A.     FRY 647 

The  photoreceptor  process  in  vision 

GEORGE    WALD 67 1 

Neural  activity  in  the  retina 

RAGN.-^R     GRANIT 693 

Central  mechanisms  of  vision 

S.     HOWARD     HARTLEY 713 

XXXI.  Central  control  of  receptors  and  sensory 
transmission  systems 

ROBERT    B.    LIVINGSTON 74 1 

Index 761 


XXV. 


XXVI. 


XXX. 


CHAPTER    I 


The  historical  development  of  neurophysiology 


MARY   A.    B.   BRAZIER      \      Ma'^sachusetts  General  Hospital,  Boston,  Massachusetts 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Early  Concepts  of  Nervous  Activity 

ENcitability  anci  Transmission  in  Nerves 

Spinal  Cord  and  Reflex  Activity 

Physiology  of  the  Brain:  Development  of  Ideas  and  Growth  of 

Experiment 
Short  List  of  Secondary  Sources 
Biographies 


EARLY  CONCEPTS  OF  NERVOUS  ACTIVITY 

IN  CONTRAST  TO  MEDICINE,  a  sciencc  demanding 
synthesis  of  observations,  experimental  physiology, 
with  its  reliance  on  analysis  and  laboratory  work,  has 
little  significant  history  before  1600.  Leaders  in 
medicine  developed  and  practiced  its  therapies  for 
many  centuries  before  they  felt  the  need  to  under- 
stand the  nature  and  functions  of  the  body's  parts  in 
any  truly  physiological  sense  and,  when  the  urge  for 
this  knowledge  first  arose,  it  was  to  come  as  mucli 
from  the  philosophers  as  from  the  healers  of  the  sick. 
Neurophysiology  (a  term  not  to  come  into  use 
until  centuries  later)  had  as  a  legacy  from  the  ancients 
only  their  speculative  inferences  and  their  primitive 
neuroanatomy.  Aristotle  had  confounded  nerves 
with  tendons  and  ligaments,  had  thought  the  brain 
bloodless  and  the  heart  supreme,  not  only  as  a  source 
of  the  nerves  but  as  the  seat  of  the  soul.  Herophilos 
and  Erisistratos  had  recognized  the  brain  as  the 
center  of  the  nervous  svstem  and  the  nerves  as  con- 
cerned both  with  sen.sation  and  movement.  However, 
preliminary  to  all  disciplines  was  the  development  of 
the  scientific  method  and  in  this  Aristotle  was  a  fore- 
runner. If  Aristotle  is  to  be  evaluated  as  a  scientist,  it 
must  be  admitted  that  he  was  almost  always  wrong  in 


every  inference  he  made  from  his  \ast  collections  of 
natural  history  and  numerous  dis.sections;  yet  in  spite 
of  the  stultifying  effect  of  the  ininujdcrate  worship 
gi\en  him  by  generations  to  follow,  he  stands  out  as  a 
pioneer  in  the  background  of  every  scientific  dis- 
cipline. He  owes  this  position  to  his  in\ention  of  a 
formal  logic,  and  although  his  system  lacked  what  the 
modern  scientist  uses  most,  namely  hypothesis  and 
induction,  his  was  a  first  step  towards  the  introduc- 
tion of  logic  as  a  tool  for  the  scientist.  Unfortunately 
Aristotle  did  not  use  his  logic  for  this  purpose  him- 
.self '  As  Francis  Bacon  put  it,  Aristotle  "did  not  con- 
sult experience  in  order  to  make  right  propositions 
and  axioms,  but  when  he  had  settled  his  system  to 
his  will,  he  twisted  experience  round,  and  made  her 
bend  to  his  system." 

In  the  .second  century  A.D.,  Galen's  experimental 
work  added  little  to  establish  the  functions  of  the 
animal  structures  he  dissected,  though  the  hypotheses 
he  suggested  were  put  forward  so  authoritatively 
that  they  remained  unchallenged  for  nearly  1500 
years.  To  the  intervening  centuries,  dominated  as 
they  were  by  the  Christian  church,  the  teleology 
implicit  in  Galen's  approach  was  attractive.  Early 
Western  acquaintance  with  his  writings  depended 
entirely  upon  Latin  translations  of  Arabic.  It  was  only 
after  the  fall  of  the  Byzantine  Empire  and  the  expul- 
sion of  the  Greek  monks  from  the  area  of  Turkish 
concjuest  that  the  Greek  language  began  to  be  read  at 

'  The  fragments  of  Aristotle's  writings  that  e.xist  (probably 
his  lecture  notes}  were  not  collected  until  more  than  lioo 
years  after  his  death.  His  Opera  were  among  the  early  scientific 
works  to  be  printed  (in  Latin,  1472),  nearly  1800  years  after  his 
death.  English  translations  (The  Works  of  Aristotle)  were  pub- 
lished by  the  Clarendon  Press,  Oxford,  in  several  volumes 
between  1909  and  1931,  edited  by  J.  A.  Smith  and  W.  A.  Ross. 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^   NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


all  generally  by  scholars  in  Western  Europe  (i,  2). 
In  the  sixteenth  century  Thomas  Linacre  (3),  physi- 
cian to  Henry  V'lII,  who  had  taught  Greek  to  Eras- 
mus at  Oxford,  translated  some  of  Galen's  works  into 
Latin  directly  from  the  Greek.  The  copies  he  gave  to 
Henrv  VIII  and  to  Cardinal  VVolsey  can  be  seen  in 
the  British  Museum.  Erasmus,  commenting  on  Lin- 
acre's  translations,  said,  "I  present  you  with  the 
works  of  Galen,  by  the  help  of  Linacre,  speaking  better 
Latin  than  ever  they  spoke  Greek." 

Galen's  emphasis,  in  spite  of  his  dissection  of  ani- 
mals, was  not  so  much  on  the  structures  he  found  as 
on  the  contents  of  the  cavities  within  them.  Function, 
according  to  his  doctrine,  was  mediated  by  humors 
which  were  respon.sible  for  all  sensation,  movement, 
desires  and  thought,  and  hence  pathology  was 
founded  on  humoral  disturbance.  The  role  of  the 
organs  of  the  body  was  to  manufacture  and  process 
these  humors.  His  teaching  about  the  nervous  system 
was  that  the  blood,  manufactured  in  the  liver  and 
carrying  in  it  natural  spirits,  flowed  to  the  heart  where 
a  change  took  place  converting  them  into  vital  spirits. 
These  travelled  to  the  reie  muahtle  (the  terminal 
branches  of  the  carotid  arteries  at  the  base  of  the 
brain)  where  they  were  changed  into  animal  spirits,- 
a  subtle  fluid  which  then  flowed  out  to  the  body 
through  hollow  nerves.  Some  of  the.se  ideas  Galen 
developed  from  those  of  his  predece.s.sors  (such  as 
Alcmaeon,  Herophilos,  Erisistratos),  some  were 
inspired  by  his  dissection  of  animals,  but  all  were 
hypothetical,    none   had   any   experimental   proof  or 

1.  Galen  (130-200  A.D.).  Opera  Omnia  (in  acdibus  Atdi  el 
Andrea  Asulani)  (in  Greek).  Venice,  1525.  5  vol. 

2.  Galen.  Opera  Omnia  (in  Greek).  Basle,   1538. 

3.  Galen.  De  Facullalibiis  naturalibus,  Latin  translation  by 
Thomas  Linacre.  London:  Pynson,  1523;  English  transla- 
tion by  A.  J.  Brock,  Loeb  Classical  Library.  London: 
Heineman,  1916. 

'  The  usage  of  the  term  animal  spirits'  throughout  the 
centuries  carries  the  connotation  of  the  Latin  anima  meaning 
soul  and  has  no  reference  to  the  modern  meaning  of  the  word 
'animal.' 

^  No  other  was  to  appear  until  the  beginning  of  the  eighteenth 
century  when  Johann  Gottfried  von  Berger  (1659-1736) 
published  his  textbook  entitled  P/iysiologa  Medica  sine  natura 
humana.  Wittenberg:  Kreusig,  1701. 

'  "Nor  lesse  Worthy  of  Commendation  are  the  Cravings.  .  . 
those  eleven  pieces  of  Anatomic  made  for  Andrea  Vessalius 
design'd  by  Calcare  the  Fleming,  an  Excellent  painter,  and 
which  were  afterwards  engraven  in  Copper  by  Valverdi  in 
little."  Evelyn,  John.  Sculpltira:  or  the  History,  and  Art  of  Chalcog- 
raphy. London,  1662.  The  reference  is  to  the  plagiarism  of  the 
Spaniard,  Juan  Valverde.  Vivae  Imagines  Partium  Corporis 
Humani.  Antwerp:  Plantin,  1566.  (His  artist  was  Becerra.) 


even  partial  support,  yet  some  of  them  were  to  last 
well  into  the  nineteenth  century. 

The  sixteenth  century  gave  to  physiology  its  first 
textbook.^  This  was  the  contribution  of  Jean  Fernel, 
physician  and  scholar,  who  in  1542  published  his 
De  Naturali  Parte  Medicinae  (4).  This  was  so  well 
received  that  it  saw  inany  editions.  In  the  ninth  of 
these  Fernel  changed  the  title  to  Medicina  (5)  and 
named  the  first  section  of  the  revised  book  Physio- 
logia.  According  to  Sherrington  (6)  this  was  the  first 
use  of  the  term  'physiology.'  There  is,  however,  a 
manuscript  in  the  Danish  Royal  Library  entitled 
Physiologus  that  deals  with  animals  and  inonsters. 
This  copy  is  an  Icelandic  version  of  an  apparently 
much-copied  treatise;  it  is  a  kind  of  bestiary.  For  some 
time  after  Fernel's  revival  of  it,  the  term  'physiology' 
was  still  used  by  most  writers  to  mean  natural  philoso- 
phy. An  example  of  this  usage  is  to  be  found  in  the  full 
title  of  Gilberd's  book  on  the  magnet  published  in 
1600.  Although  still  grounded  in  a  classification  de- 
rived froin  the  four  elements  of  the  ancients,  Fernel's 
physiology  nevertheless  shows  dawning  recognition 
of  some  of  the  automatic  movements  which  we  now 
know  to  be  reflexly  initiated  for,  although  only  the 
voluntary  muscles  were  known  to  him,  he  realized  that 
sometimes  they  moved  independently  of  the  will. 

Before  the  seventeenth  century  opened,  a  technical 
achievement  in  another  field  laid  a  foundation  on 
which  physiology  was  to  spread.  Lagging  about  50 
years  after  the  invention  of  printing  came  the  develop- 
ment of  copper  plate  engraving  and  accurate  repro- 
ductions of  anatomists'  drawings  became  more 
widely  distributed.  Supreine,  however,  ainong  the 
woodcuts  contemporary  with  the  early  engravings 
were  those  made  from  the  drawings  of  Jan  Stephen  of 
Calcar  for  the  anatoinical  studies  of  Vesalius  (7^9). 
These,  published  in  1543,  were  to  draw  the  praise  of 
John  Evelyn  in  his  treatise  on  chalcography.^  After 

4.  Fernel,  Jean  (1497- 1558).  De  Naturali  Parte  Medicinae. 
Paris:  Simon  de  Colines,   1542. 

5.  Fernel,  J.  Medicina.  Paris:  Wechsel,  1554.  P/iysiologia, 
translated  into  French  by  Charles  de  Saint  Germain, 
Les  VII  Livres  de  la  Physiologic,  composes  en  Latin  par  Messire 
Jean  Fernel.  Paris:  Guignard,   1655. 

6.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  The  Endeavour  of  Jean  Fernel.  Cam- 
bridge: Cambridge,  1946. 

7.  Vesalius,  Andreas  (1514-1564).  De  Humani  Corporis 
Fabrica.  Basle:  Oporinus,  1543;  translated  into  English 
by  J.  B.  de  C.  M.  Saunders  and  C.  D.  OMallcy.  New 
York:  Schuman,  1947. 

8.  Vesalius,  A.  Epitome.  Basle:  Oporinus,  translated  into 
English  by  L.   R.  Rind.  New  York:  Macmillan,   1949. 

9.  Vesalius,  .\.   Tabulae  Sex.  Venice,   1538. 


THE   HISTORICAL  DEVELOPMENT  OF  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


centuries  in  which  human  dissection  could  onl\'  be 
done  relatively  furtively,  a  more  liberal  view  had 
grown  up  in  Italy  and  among  a  number  of  con- 
temporary anatomists,  Vesalius  is  pre-eminent.  In 
themselves,  however,  with  the  exception  of  an  experi- 
ment showing  that  the  nerve  sheath  is  not  vital  for 
conduction,  his  studies  made  no  contribution  to  the 
dynamics  of  function.  Although  an  opponent  of 
Galen  and  an  exposer  of  his  anatomical  errors, 
Vesalius  had  no  more  satisfactory  concept  of  nervous 
activity  to  offer  than  that  of  animal  spirits  flowing 
from  the  brain  down  pipe-like  nerves  to  the  muscles. 
Yet  for  the  study  of  the  nervous  system,  as  for  other 
branches  of  physiology,  the  publication  of  De  Humam 
Corporis  Fabrica  is  the  outstanding  contribution  of 
the  sixteenth  century,  the  earlier  chalk  drawings  of 
Leonardo  Da  Vinci  (1452-1519)  not  being  widely 
known  to  his  contemporaries.  The  major  contribu- 
tions of  Vesalius  were  not  in  physiology  but  in  anat- 
omy and  in  the  demonstration  that  Galen  was  capa- 
ble of  error  (though  he  himself  was  not  without  error). 

At  the  opening  of  the  seventeenth  century  the  im- 
portant event  for  all  science  was  the  appearance 
(in  1600)  of  William  Giiberd's*  classic  book  De 
Magnete  (10,  11).  The  significance  of  this  work  was 
not  only  as  a  landmark  for  the  future  of  the  physical 
sciences  and  of  electrophy.siology  through  its  dawning 
recognition  of  a  difference  between  electricity  and 
magnetism;  it  was  the  first  book  to  advocate  empirical 
methods  and  in  this  way  heralded  the  scientific 
ferment  of  the  eighteenth  century.  If  one  overlooks 
the  last  two  chapters  oi  De  Magnete,  the  book  is  revolu- 
tionary in  its  experimental  approach.  It  stood  out 
alone  in  an  age  when  scholasticism  was  concerned 
with  classification  on  qualitative  lines  without  meas- 
urement and  without  validation.  Authoritative  state- 
ments of  the  ancients  were  the  guides,  and  induction 
from  experiment  was  virtually  unknown.  Gilberd's 
book  makes  a  plea  for  "trustworthy  experiments  and 
demonstrated  arguments"  to  replace  "the  probable 
guesses  and  opinions  of  the  ordinary  professors  of 
philosophy." 

Gilberd  was  physician  to  Queen  Elizabeth  (whom 


he  only  just  survi\-cdj  and  a  sketch  identified  as  a 
portrait  of  him  appears  in  the  contemporary  draw- 
ing (now  in  the  British  Museum)  made  by  William 
Camden,  the  Court  Herald,  of  her  funeral  proces- 
sion in  1603.  A  contemporary  oil  portrait  of  him 
painted  in  1591  has  been  lost  and  remains  to  us  only 
in  engravings.  Gilberd  was  born  and  lived  part  of 
his  life  in  his  father's  house  in  Colchester  in  East 
Anglia;  a  portion  of  this  house  still  stands  and,  at 
the  time  of  writing,  is  being  restored.  This  flowering 
of  the  .scientific  method  came  during  the  golden  age 
of  Elizabethan  England;  among  Gilberd's  contem- 
poraries were  Shakespeare,  Walter  Raleigh,  Philip 
Sydney,  John  Donne,  Christopher  Marlow  and 
Francis  Bacon. 

Francis  Bacon  has  a  place  in  the  history  of  all 
.sciences,  for  he  took  scientific  method  a  step  farther, 
to  observation  he  added  induction  and  to  inference  he 
added  verification.  Scientists  before  him  were  content 
with  performing  an  experiment  in  order  to  make 
an  observation;  from  this  oijservation  a  series  of 
propositions  would  follow,  each  being  derived  from  its 
predecessor,  not  by  experiment  but  by  logic.  (Bacon 
somewhat  unjustly  criticizes  Gilberd  for  proceeding 
in  this  way.)  Bacon's  contribution  to  scientific  method 
was  to  urge,  in  addition,  the  rigorous  application  of 
a  special  kind  of  inductive  reasoning  proceeding 
from  the  accumulation  of  a  number  of  particular 
facts  to  the  demonstration  of  their  interrelation 
and  hence  to  a  general  conclusion.  This  was  in- 
deed a  new  instrument,  a  Novum  Organum  (12).  By  its 
application  he  overthrew  reliance  on  authority  of 
the  ancients  and  opened  the  way  for  planned  experi- 
ment. Although  he  had  no  place  in  his  method  for 
the  working  hypothesis,  and  his  forms  of  induction 
and  deduction  are  scarcely  those  of  the  modern 
methodology,  they  were  of  considerable  influence  in 
its  development.  The  intelligent  lines  of  Bacon's 
face  can  be  seen  in  his  portraits.  John  Aubrey  (13) 
tells  us  that  he  "had  a  delicate,  lively  hazel  eie"  and 
that  "Dr.  Harvey  told  me  it  was  like  the  eie  of  a 
viper." 

The  first  major  work  in  physiology  exemplifying 


10.  Gilberd,  William  (1540  (or  1544)- 1603).  De  Magnete, 
Magnetisque  corporibus;  et  de  mag?io  magnete  lellure;  Physio- 
logica  nova  plurimis  et  argumentis  et  experimentis  demonstrata. 
London:  Peter  Short,  1600;  translated  into  English  by 
the  Gilbert  Club,  William  Gilbert  of  Colchester,  physician 
of  London.  London;  Chiswick  Press,  igoo. 

11.  Ibid.  (2nd  ed.)  (posthumous).  Gotzianio  in  Stettin,  1633. 
This  book,  far  rarer  than  the  first  edition,  carries  more 
plates  than  the  original,  and  has  some  additions  by 
Wolfgang  Lochmann  of  Pomerania  (1594- 1643). 


12.  Bacon,    Francis    (1561-1626).    .Novum    Organum.     1620; 
translated  into  English  by  Kitchin.  Oxford,  1855. 

13.  Aubrey,  John  (1626- 1697).   Brief  Lives  set  Down  i66g- 
i6g6,  edited  by  Andrew  Clark.   Clarendon  Press,    1898, 

vol.  2. 


'  The  spelling  of  Gilberd's  name  follows  the  form  seen  on  his 
portrait  and  memorial  tablet;  his  name  on  his  book  is  spelled 
Gilbert. 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPH^'SIOLOGY    1 


FIG.  I.  Portrait  of  William  Gilberd  from  an  oil  painting  on  wood,  found  by  Silvanus  P.  Thompson 
in  an  antiquary's  shop.  The  artist  and  the  authenticity  of  the  date  on  this  portrait  are  unknown. 
The  portrait  is  now  in  the  possession  of  Miss  Helen  G.  Thompson,  by  whose  courtesy  it  is  repro- 
duced here.  The  photograph  of  Tymperleys,'  Gilberd's  home  at  Colchester,  was  taken  in  1957 
when  the  house  was  undergoing  extensive  restoration.  A  portion  only  of  the  house  dates  from  Gil- 
berd's time.  (Photograph  by  courtesy  of  Dr.  G.  Burniston  Brown.) 


Bacon's  methodolos;y  was  iioi  on  the  nervous  .system 
but  on  the  circulation  of  the  blood.  Harvey's  magnif- 
icent treatise  De  Motu  Cordis  (14)  was  a  model  for 
workers  in  all  branches  of  physiology  to  follow.  This 
small  book  (it  has  only  72  pages)  was  the  first  major 
treatment  of  a  physiological  subject  in  dynamic 
rather  than  static  terins.  By  experiment  Harvey  dis- 
proved the  Galenist  doctrine  that  the  motion  of  the 
blood  in  the  arterial  and  venous  systems  was  a  tidal 
ebb  and  flow,  independent  except  for  .some  leakage 
through  'pores'  in  the  interventricular  septum.  By 
further  designed  experiments  Harvey  proved  his  own 
hypothesis  "that  the  blood  in  the  animal  body  is  im- 
pelled in  a  circle,  and  is  in  a  state  of  ceaseless  motion." 
Harvey  had  advanced   this  hypothesis  in    1616   but, 

14.  H.^RVEV,  VViLLI.aiM  (i  578- 1 657).  Exercttatin  analomica  de 
motu  cordis  et  sanguinis  in  animiilihus.  Frankfurt :  Fitzeri, 
1628;  translated  into  English  by  VVillius  and  Keys,  Car- 
diac Classics,   1 94 1,  p.   19. 

15.  H.ARVEV,  \V.  Praelecliones  anatomiae  universalis.  London: 
Churchill,  1886.  (Reprint  of  Harvey's  Lumleian  lecture 
1616.) 


as  a  forerunner  of  modern  scientific  method,  had 
proceeded  to  verify  it  before  publishing  his  book. 
But  even  this  triumph  of  the  empirical  method  did 
not  unseat  in  Harvey's  thinking  the  belief  in  a  soul 
located  in  the  blood  ('anima  ipsa  esse  sanguis')  (15). 
Harvey  was  Galenist  enough  to  accept  the  rete  mirabile 
as  the  destination  of  the  blood  within  the  craniitm, 
although  doubt  as  to  its  existence  in  man  had  already 
been  raised  by  Berengario  da  Carpi  (16,  i  7)  a  hun- 
dred years  before.  Harvey  (18)  had  his  own  \'iews 
of  nervous  function.  "I  believe,"  he  said,  "that  in  the 
nerves  there  is  no  progression  of  spirits,  but  irradia- 
tion; and  that  the  actions  from  which  sensation  and 

16.  Berengario  da  Carpi,  Giacomo  (1470- 1550).  Com- 
menlaria  cum  amplissimus  addilionibus  super  analomia  Mun- 
dtni.  Bologna :  Benedictis,  1 52 1 . 

17.  Berengario  da  Carpi,  G.  Isagogae  breves,  perlucidae.  In: 
Analomiam  hurnani  corporis,  ad  suorum  scholasticorum  preces 
in  lucem  edilae.  Bologna,  1522;  translated  into  English  by 
H.  Jackson,  under  the  title  A  description  of  the  Bnd\  of  Mnn, 
being  a  practical  Anatomy.  London,  1664. 

18.  Harvey,  W.  Praelecliones  Analo?nwe  Universalis,  autotype 
reproduction  edition.   Philadelphia:  Cole,    1886. 


THE   HISTORICAL  DEVELOPMENT   OF   NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


FIG.  2.  Borclli  and  one  of  his  sketches  to  show  the  center  of 
gravity  of  man  when  carrying  a  load.  (From  BorelU,  G.A.  De 
Mntu  Animalium,  2nd  ed.,  Leydon :  Gaesbeeck,  1685.) 

motion  result  are  brought  about  as  light  is  in  air, 
perhaps  as  the  flu.x  and  reflu.x  of  the  sea." 

That  nerves  might  play  a  role  in  the  working  of 
the  heart  as  a  mechanical  pump  was  first  suggested 
by  Borelli  the  Neapolitan,  professor  of  mathematics  at 
Pisa  and  later  at  Florence,  who  applied  the  reasoning 
of  his  discipline  to  physiology  and  e\olved  mechani- 
cal models  for  various  bodily  functions.  His  concept 
of  the  innervation  of  muscle  was  an  initiation  by  the 
nervous  fluid  ('succus  nervcus')  of  a  fermentation  in 
the  mu.scle  swelling  it  into  contraction,  for  there  were 
still  many  years  to  go  before  a  dynamic  concept  of 
muscle  was  to  emerge  in  spite  of  Harvey's  demon- 
strations on  the  heart.  Peripheral  muscles  were  still 
regarded  as  passive  structures  rather  like  balloons  to 
be  inflated  by  nervous  fluid  or  gaseous  spirits  reach- 
ing them  through  canals  in  the  nerves.  Borelli,  by 
an  ingenious  experiment  in  which  he  submerged  a 
struggling  animal  in  water  and  then  slit  its  muscles, 
demonstrated  that  the  spirits  could  not  Ije  gaseous 
since  no  bubbles  appeared  in  spite  of  the  violent 
contractions.  It  was  this  experiment  that  led  him  to 
the  suggestion  of  a  liquid  medium  from  the  nerve, 
mixing  in  the  muscle  to  cause  a  contraction  by  ex- 
plosi\e  fermentation  ('ebuUitio  et  displosio')  (19). 

Giovanni  Alphonso  Borelli  was  a  member  of  the 
group  of  experimental  scientists  banded  together  in 
the  Accademia  del  Cimento  under  tlie  patronage  of 
the  .science-loving  Medici  brothers  in  Florence.  This 
small  .scientific  society,  successor  to  the  Lincei,  existed 
for  only  a  decade  but  was  typical  of  the  independent 

ig.  Borelli,  Giovanni  .Alfonso  (1608-1679).  De  molu  ani- 
malium (pubUshed  posthumously).  Rome:  Bernado,  1680- 
I ;  a  small  section  has  been  translated  into  English  by 
Michael  Foster.  Lectures  on  the  History  of  Physiology.  Cam- 
bridge: Cambridge,  1901. 


groups  centered  on  laboratory  experiment  that  were 
to  spring  up  in  independence  of  the  universities  where 
the  scholars  had  still  not  looked  up  from  their  books. 
Few  as  they  were  (there  were  only  nine  members) 
these  laboratory  scientists  of  the  Accademia  were  to 
have  a  far-reaching  though  delayed  influence  on 
European  thought,  for  in  the  final  year  of  the  acad- 
emy's existence  they  published  their  proceedings  (20). 
Founded  entirely  on  empirical  methodology,  this  was 
a  truly  scientific  text.  It  was,  however,  written  in 
Italian  although  soon  translated  into  English,  and  it 
did  not  reach  the  scientific  world  at  large  until  Petrus 
van  Musschenbroek  of  Leydeii  made  a  Latin  transla- 
tion (21).  It  was  this  book  that,  for  example,  influ- 
enced Stephen  Hales  so  greatly  in  his  experimental 
work.  The  volume  included  only  one  series  on  animal 
experimentation,  but  almost  all  the  rest  deals  with 
the  physics  which  are  basic  to  the  work  a  physiologist 
does  in  his  laboratory. 

To  his  contemporary,  Descartes,  Borelli  owed  his 
application  of  mathematics  to  muscular  action.  This 
pungent  philosopher,  who  rarely  did  an  experiment, 
wrote  a  text  that  was  to  influence  all  experimenters, 
The  Discourse  on  Method  (22).  It  is  not  experimental 
method  that  he  discusses,  i)ut  his  own  method  of 
thought,  his  theory  of  knowledge."  Scientists  had 
just  begun  to  look  around  them  to  olaserve  nature 
and  to  let  the  statements  about  her  by  the  ancients 
lie  in  the  books  when  they  had  to  meet  a  new  and 
brilliant  challenge;  mathematics  was  the  tool  they 
were  to  use.  Mathematics  would  not  only  elucidate 
the  laboratory  experiment  but  would  provide  the 
basis  for  an  all-embracing  theory  of  science. 

This  great  man  bred  in  the  gentle  landscape  of 
Touraine  was  to  devote  his  life  to  a  search  for  the 
truth,  .seeking  for  himself  a  quiet  environment  for 
free   thinking.'   This   he  found   for   25   years   in   the 

20.  Saggi  (It  naturali  esperienrji  fattr  nell  Aecadeniui  del  Cimento, 
edited  by  L.  Magalotti.  Florence,  1667;  translated  into 
English  by  Richard  Waller.  Essayes  of  Natural  Experiments 
made  in  the  Accademie  del  Cimento.  London,    1684. 

21.  VAN  Musschenbroek,  Petrus  (1692-1761).  Testamina 
Experimentortum  Naturaliuin  captoruni  in  Accademia  del  Ci- 
mento. Leyden,  1731. 

22.  Descartes,  R.  Discours  de  la  Methode.  1637 ^  English  trans- 
lation by  E.  S.  Haldane  and  G.  R.  T.  Ross.  Philosophical 
Works  of  Descartes.  Cambridge :  Cambridge,  1 904. 

*  "Methode  de  bien  conduire  sa  raison,  pour  trouver  la 
verite  dans  les  sciences." 

'  "Cum  nil  dignum  apud  homines  scientia  sua  invenisset, 
eremum  ut  Democritus  aliique  vcri  Philosophi  elegit  sibi 
juxta  Egmundum  in  HoUandia,  sibique  solitarius  in  villula  per 
25  annos  remansit,  admirandaque  multa  meditatione  sua 
detexit"  (Borel,  p.  9). 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  3.  Rene  Descartes  and  his  concept  of  the  pineal  gland.  The  photograph  is  from  the  portrait 
by  Franz  Hals  in  the  Louvre,  and  the  diagram  is  taken  from  de  la  Forge,  Louis.  Traili  de  I' Esprit 
de  I'Homme,  de  ses  Facultez,  de  ses  Fonclions,  et  de  son  Union  avec  le  Corps.  SuiranI  les  principes  de  Mr. 
Descartes.  Geneva :  Bousquet,  1725. 


village  of  Egmond  in  liberal  Holland,  though  even 
here  he  could  not  entirely  escape  lieing  hounded  by 
bigots.  The  mistake  he  made  that  the  world  regrets 
was  to  leave  a  milieu  so  congenial  to  his  philosophic 
nature  for  the  cold  of  Sweden  and  the  exacting  de- 
mands of  Queen  Christina.  There,  within  a  year,  he 
died.  His  striking  face  with  the  intelligent  eyes  and 
quizzical  eyebrow  has  been  preserved  for  us  in  the 
fine  portrait  by  Franz  Hals  that  hangs  in  the  Loux-re. 
A  great  man  has  many  lives'  written  about  him 
but  those  set  down  by  his  contemporaries  usually 
have  a  special  flavor.  In  the  case  of  Descartes,  the 
short  account  of  his  life  and  his  philosophy  written 
by  Borel  (23)  (the  inicroscopist)  in  1669  gi\es  one  the 
feeling  of  bridging  the  centuries.  Borel  gives  a  list  of 
the  manuscripts  found  in  Stockholm  at  Descartes's 
death  in  1650,  including  the  early  treatise  he  wrote 
on  music  when  he  was  only  22.  Several  of  his  letters 
were  found,  some  of  which  Borel  reproduces.  The 
letters  date  from  1632  and  give  an  intimate  glimp.se  of 
the  struggle  Descartes  had  to  face  in  overcoming  re- 
sistance to  his  theories  among  some  of  his  con- 
temporaries. 

23.  Borel,   Pierre  (1620- 1689).    Vitae  Renati  Cartesii,  Summt 
Philosophi  Compendium.  Frankfurt:  Sigismund,  1676. 

*  "It  is  an  error  to  suppose  the  soul  supplies  the  body  with 
its  heat  and  its  movements."  Passions  de  I'Ame,  Article  5. 


Descartes  (24,  25),  having  become  convinced  that 
in  mathematics  lay  the  tool  for  a  unified  theory  of  all 
science,  had  now  to  explain  its  role  in  physiology.  It 
followed  logically  that  the  animal  body  and  all  its 
workings  was  a  machine,  the  operation  of  this  machine 
being  directed  from  a  control  tower.  In  the  brain  with 
its  bilateral  development,  the  singly  represented 
pineal  body  was  chosen  by  Descartes  to  play  this 
master  role  and  (in  man)  it  was  given  the  added 
responsibility  of  housing  the  soul.  In  the  concept  of 
the  body  as  a  machine,  energized  not  by  an  iniina- 
terial  aninia*  but  by  the  external  world  impinging  on 
it,  lies  a  germ  of  the  idea  of  reflex  activity. 

To  coming  generations  of  neurophysiologists  Des- 
cartes bequeathed  the  notion  that  impressions  from 
the  external  world  were  conveyed  by  material  animal 
spirits  to  the  ventricles  and  there  directed  by  the 
pineal  gland  into  those  outgoing  tubular  nerves  that 
could  carry  them  to  the  part  of  the  body  the  subse- 
quent action  of  which  would  be  the  appropriate  one. 
In  animals  this  was  presumed  to  be  a  purely  mechani- 
cal action,  but  in  man  the  soul,  resident  in  the  pineal, 
could  have  soine  say  in  the  direction  taken  by  this 

24.  Descartes,  Rene  (1596-1650).  Passioru  de  fAnie. 
.-\msterdam,  1649. 

25.  Descartes,  R.  De  homine  figuris,  et  latinate  donatur  a 
Florentio  Schuyl,  posthumous  Latin  version  by  Schuyl. 
Leyden:  Moyardum  &  Leffen,  1662;  first  French  edition, 
Traile  de  I'Homme,  1664;  second  French  edition,  1677. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


central  relay.  Descartes  recognized,  however,  that 
perhaps  some  of  these  actions  lay  outside  the  control 
of  the  will,  citing  as  examples  involuntary  blinking 
and  the  withdrawal  of  the  hand  on  burning. 

To  neurophysiologists  Descartes  bequeathed  an- 
other seed — what  was  later  to  be  known  as  the  re- 
ciprocal innervation  of  antagonist  muscles.  In  order 
to  ensure  that  while  animal  spirits  were  flowing 
into  one  set  of  muscles  the  opposing  set  should  relax, 
he  argued  that  the  latter  must  have  their  supply  of 
spirits  blocked  and  he  postulated  that  this  must  be 
eflTected  by  valves.  Whether  or  not  he  was  influenced 
in  his  thinking  by  Harvey's  explanation  of  the 
valves  of  the  veins  is  not  known,  although  he  was 
certainly  aware  of,  and  had  commented  on,  Harvey's 
discoveries.^  Descartes  was  a  member  of  what  a  sub- 
sequent irreverent  generation  was  to  call  'the  bal- 
loonists.'  Apparently  unaware  of  Borelli's  experiments, 
he  thought  the  animal  spirits  to  be  "like  a  wind  or  a 
verv  subtle  flame"  and  that  "when  they  flow  into  a 
muscle  they  cause  it  to  become  stiff  and  swollen,  just 
as  air  in  a  balloon  makes  it  hard  and  stretches  the 
substance  in  which  it  is  contained." 

A  young  contemporary  of  Descartes,  though  less 
directly  influenced  bs'  him  than  was  Borelli,  was  Wil- 
liam Croone  who  was  working  on  muscle  action.  He 
too  thought  that  the  nervous  'juice'  must  interact  in 
some  way  with  the  muscle  (26).  The  "spiritous  liquid" 
flowed  in,  mixed  with  "the  nourishing  juice  of  the 
muscle,"  and  then  the  muscle  "swell'd  like  a  Bladder 
blown  up.  "  Later  (27}  Croone  was  to  modify  this  to 
a  number  of  small  bladders  for  each  muscle  fiber. 
Just  as  Borelli  had  been  a  founding  member  of  a 
scientific  society,  so  was  Croone.  He  was  one  of  the 
original  group  who  in  England  formed  the  Royal 
Society,  a  society  which  unlike  the  Cimento  has  con- 
tinued to  flourish  and  in  which  to  this  day  eminent 
.scientists  not  only  discuss  but  demonstrate  their  ex- 
periments before  the  members.  The  Royal  Society 
has  several  distinguished  lectureships,  among  which 
is  the  Croonian  Lecture  founded  by  the  widow  of 
William  Croone. 

The  Royal  Society  of  London  received  its  charter  in 
1662,  being  founded  for  the  promotion  of  "Natural 
Knowledge,'  and  it  numbered  among  the  founding 
members  many  who.se  contributions  are  fundamental 

26.  Croone,  William  (1633-1684).  De  raiione  motus  muscu- 
lorum  (published   anonymously).    London:   Hayes,    1664. 

■27.  Croone,  W.  An  Hypothesis  of  the  Structure  of  the  Muscle,  and 
the  Reason  of  its  Contraction.  Hooke's  Philosophical  Collec- 
tions, No.    II.   London,    1675. 


to  physiolog)-.  The  mo\ing  spirit  was  Robert  Bovle, 
the  'father  of  chemistry'  (whose  first  published  work 
was,  however,  on  Seraphick  Love).  Famous  for  his  law 
(28)  of  gaseous  pressures,  he  made  his  most  directly 
physiological  experiments  on  the  respiration  of  ani- 
mals. It  was  still  many  years  before  physiologists  were 
to  elucidate  the  efTects  of  anoxia  on  the  nervous 
system,  and  another  hundred  years  were  to  pass  before 
Priestley's  and  Lavoisier's  work  on  oxygen,  but  Boyle, 
by  using  an  ingenious  compression  chamber,  demon- 
strated that  air  is  essential  for  life.  Almost  unnoticed 
at  the  time,  but  since  then  perhaps  overpraised,  were 
the  observations  of  John  Mayow  (29)  on  the  chem- 
istry of  respiration.  His  publication  preceded  (al- 
though his  work  was  contemporary  with)  the  some- 
what similar  experiments  of  the  Accademia  del 
Cimento. 

In  the  early  seventeenth  century  emphasis  on  the 
search  for  a  chemical  foundation  for  living  phe- 
nomena characterized  for  the  most  part  work  in 
Holland  and  England  in  contrast  to  the  physical  and 
mathematical  approach  of  the  Italians  and  the 
French.  The  two  contrasting  .schools  of  thought  were 
long  to  be  known  by  the  clumsy  names  of  the  iatro- 
chemical  and  iatromechanical  schools.  latrochemis- 
try,  on  the  rather  shaky  foundations  given  to  it  by 
van  Helmont  (1577-1644)  and  by  Sylvius  (de  La  Boe) 
(1614-1672),  provided  the  approach  to  the  study  of 
the  nervous  system  of  Thomas  Willis,  Sedleian  Pro- 
fessor of  Natural  Philosophy  at  Oxford  (30).  Willis, 
whose  clinical  achievements  outshone  his  scientific 
acumen,  is  recognized  in  neurology  for  his  description 
of  the  circle  of  Willis  and  his  dissection  of  the  spinal 
accessory  nerve.  (Galen  had  identified  only  seven 
pairs  of  cranial  nerves.)  Willis  was  a  close  colleague  at 
Oxford  of  Richard  Lower,  the  Cornishman,  champion 
of  the  theory  that  spirits  flowing  into  the  heart  from 

28.  Boyle,  Robert  (162  7-1 691).  .\ew  experiments  physico- 
mechamcal,  touching  the  spring  of  the  air,  and  its  effects,  made, 
for  the  most  part,  in  a  new  pneumatical  engine.  Oxford :  VV. 
Hall,  1660. 

29.  Mayow,  John  (1645-1679).  Tractus  Duo,  quorum  prior 
agit  De  Respiratione :  alter  De  Radutiones.  O.xford:  Hall,  1668. 

30.  Willis,  Thomas  (1621-1675).  Cerebri  anatome:  cui  accessit 
nervorum  descriptio  et  usus.  (Z)f  systemate  nervosa  in  genere"), 
illustrated  by  Sir  Christopher  Wren.  London:  Flesher, 
1664;  translated  into  English  by  S.  Pordage.  London: 
Dring,  Harper  and  Leigh,   1683. 


'  Letter  to  Mersenne  dated    1632,   quoted   in   Oeuvres  Com- 
pletes de  Descartes,  edition  of  Adam  and  Tannery,  Paris;  Cerf, 

1897-1910,  vol.  n,  p.  127. 


8 


HANDBO(iK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


its  nerves  were  what  caused  it  to  beat  (31).  Lower's 
more  spectacular  achievement  was  the  apparent 
transfusion  of  blood,  first  in  dog  and  then  in  man  (32, 
33).  We  are  surprised  today  that  the  man  survived  as 
long  as  he  did,  for  the  blood  donor  was  a  sheep. 

Thomas  Willis  had  added  to  the  prevalent  Galenic 
ideas  of  nervous  function  the  concept  that  the  soul 
had  two  parts  which  he  likened  to  a  flame  in  the  vital 
fluid  of  the  blood  and  a  light  in  the  nervous  juice. 
When  they  met  in  the  muscle,  they  formed  a  highly 
explosive  mixture  which  inflated  the  muscle.  Yet  even 
before  the  seventeenth  century  had  run  out,  a  voice 
was  raised  against  such  visionary  explanations.  Sten- 
sen  (34)'  '^he  great  Danish  anatomist,  writing  from 
Florence  in  1667,  stated  unequivocally  that  "Animal 
spirits,  the  more  subtle  part  of  the  blood,  the  vapour 
of  blood,  and  the  juice  of  the  ner\-es,  these  are  names 
used  by  many,  but  they  are  mere  words,  meaning 
nothing." 

The  seventeenth  century,  or  grand  siecle  as  it  was 
known  to  Europe,  had  been  gloriously  opened  by 
the  De  Magnete  and  gone  on  to  the  achievements  of 
Galileo,  Kepler,  Huygens,  Leibniz  and  Newton,  and, 
although  these  were  essentially  achievements  in 
mathematics,  physics  and  astronomy,  all  branches  of 
science  were  fermenting  with  the  implications  of  these 
disco\'eries.  The  break  with  dogma  was  now  more 
than  a  crack,  though  the  Index  Librorum  Prolnb- 
itorutn  fought  a  delaying  action.  The  men  of  the 
arts  were  liberal  in  their  championship  of  the  scientists. 
John  Milton's  Areopagitica  (35)  is  a  clarion  call  for 
freedom  of  knowledge  and  distribution  of  books. 
Milton  was  a  young  contemporary  of  Galileo  and 
went  to  see  him  in  his  old  age.  There  is  a  poignancy 
about  this  visit  to  the  old  blind  astronomer  from  the 
poet  about  to  become  blind. 

The  students  of  the  nervous  system  had  the  hardest 
fight   against   dogma  for   in   their  province   lay   the 

31.  Lower,  Richard  (1631-1691).  Tractatus  de  Corde  item  de 
Motu  &  Colore  Sanguinis  el  Chyli  cum  Transitu.  London: 
AUestry,  i66g;  English  translation  by  K.  J.  Franklin. 
Early  Science  in  Oxford.  Oxford,   1932,  \ol.  g. 

32.  Lower,  R.  The  method  observed  in  transfusing  the  blood 
out  of  one  live  animal  into  another.  Phil.  Trans,  i  :  353, 
1665-6. 

33.  Lower,  R.  and  E.  King.  An  account  of  the  experiment 
of  transfusion,  practised  upon  a  man  in  London.  P/ul. 
Trans.  2:  1557,  1667. 

34.  Stensen,  Nicholas  (1638- 1686).  Elernentorum  myologiae 
.'Specimen.  Florence:  Stella,  1667,  p.  83. 

35.  Milton,  John  (1608-1674).  Areopagitica.  A  speech  for  the 
Liberty  of  Unlicensed  Printing  to  the  Parliament  of  England. 
1644. 


structures  most  suspect  as  being  the  guardians  of 
man's  soul.  But  ranked  behind  them  and  influential  on 
them  were  some  of  the  greatest  philosophers  of  their 
time.  Prominent  among  the.se  was  Locke  (36),  the 
father  of  empiricism.  Born  in  the  West  of  England  and 
trained  as  a  physician,  this  man  with  his  colorless 
personality  and  his  clumsy  prose  was  to  channel  the 
efforts  of  the  next  several  generations  of  workers  on 
the  nervous  system  into  a  .search  for  the  physiology 
of  the  mind.  For  his  Essay  on  Humane  Understanding 
he  received  immediate  recognition  and  monetary 
reward,  obtaining  for  it  more  than  was  paid  to  John 
Milton  for  Paradise  Lost. 

Straddling  like  a  colo.ssus  the  division  between  the 
seventeenth  and  eighteenth  centuries  is  Newton, 
friend  and  correspondent  of  Locke,  though  to  .scien- 
tists it  is  perhaps  a  bit  disappointing  to  find  that  the 
subject  of  their  correspondence  was  the  interpretation 
of  the  New  Testament  (biblical  history  was  a  life-long 
interest  of  Newton).  Newton's  insight  into  the  move- 
ment and  forces  of  nature  led  him  to  make  some 
tentative  suggestions  about  the  working  of  the  nerv- 
ous system,  and  these  were  noted  by  the  physiologists 
of  the  time.  There  is  scarcely  a  single  neurophysiolo- 
gist  of  the  eighteenth  century  who  does  not  explicitly 
attempt  to  align  his  findings  with  these  conjectures 
of  Newton. 

In  the  General  Scholium  (37)  which  he  added  to 
the  second  edition  of  the  Principia  (26  years  after  its 
first  publication),  Newton  included  a  speculation. 
This  was  the  idea  of  an  all-pervading  elastic  aether 
"exceedingly  more  rare  and  subtle  than  the  air," 
which  he  again  suggested  in  the  form  of  a  question  in 
the  .series  of  Queries  added  to  the  second  English  edi- 
tion of  his  Opticks  (38).  Applying  this  suggestion  to 
the  nervous  system,  he  said,  "I  suppose  that  the  Capil- 
lamenta  of  the  Nerves  are  each  of  them  solid  and 
uniform,  that  the  \ibrating  Motion  of  the  Aetherial 
Medium  may  be  propagated  along  them  from  one 
End  to  the  other  uniformly,  and  without  interrup- 
tion. .  .  ."  It  is  easy  to  understand  how  eagerly  such  a 
statement  would  be  received  by  those  who  accepted 
the  idea  of  a  nervous  principle  running  down  the 
nerves  but  were  worried  that  they  knew  of  no  fluid 
sufficiently  swift  and  invisible.  Newton's  rather  sketchy 
suggestion  was  therefore  eagerly  embraced  by  many 
of  his  contemporaries,  one  of  whom,  Bryan  Robinson, 

36.  Locke,   John   (163J-1704).   An  Essay   concerning    Humane 
Understanding.  London:  Holt,    1690. 

37.  Newton,    Isaac   (1642-1727).    Principia.    London:    1687; 
edition  with  General  .Scholium,   171 3. 

38.  Newton,  L  Opticks  (2nd  ed.,  24th  Query).  London:  1717. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


Regius  Professor  of  Physic  at  the  L'ni\ersity  of  Duljlin, 
even  went  so  far  as  to  claim  that  "Sir  Isaac  Newton 
discovered  the  Causes  of  Muscular  Motion  and  Secre- 
tion" (39). 

At  the  opening  of  the  eigthteenth  century  the  sci- 
ence of  the  nervous  system  had  reached  diflferent  levels 
in  the  various  countries  of  Europe.  In  Germany  in 
the  first  half  of  the  century  the  Thirty  Years  War 
had  brought  science  almost  to  a  standstill,  and  in  the 
fields  of  chemistry  and  physiology  this  stagnation  de- 
veloped into  a  retrogression  owing  to  the  emergence 
of  an  extremely  influential  figure,  Georg  Ernst  Stahl. 
In  opposition  to  both  the  chemical  and  mathematical 
schools,  Stahl  set  back  the  clock  by  the  reintroduction 
of  an  immaterial  anima  which  he  held  to  be  the  sole 
activating  principle  of  the  body  parts  (40).  The 
latter  were  regarded  as  having  no  dynamic  properties 
of  their  own,  being  essentially  passive  structures. 
Since  the  search  for  an  immaterial  agent  lies  outside 
the  scope  of  science,  Stahl's  doctrines,  promulgated 
with  arrogance  and  dogmatism,  virtually  extinguished 
experimental  inquiry  among  his  followers.  Yet  even 
writers  sympathetic  to  his  viewpoint  granted  that  in 
attempting  to  follow  his  arguments  one  became  "in- 
volved in  a  labyrinth  of  metaphysical  subtlety"  (41). 
The  metaphysical  approach  of  Stahl  later  came 
under  criticism  from  Vicq  d'Azyr  (42)  who  suggested 
that  the  invention  of  an  imaginary  soul  to  resolve  those 
phenomena  that  could  not  yet  be  explained  by  the 
laws  of  physics  and  chemistry  was  merely  a  cloak  for 
ignorance,  van  Helmont  did  not  escape  the  same 
criticism. 

In  opposition  to  humoral  or  vitalistic  concepts  of 
nervous  and  muscular  activity  was  a  prominent 
champion  of  a  'solidist'  theory,  Giorgio  Baglivi.  This 
young  man,  whom  Pope  Innocent  XII  had  appointed  to 
be  profes.sor  of  the  theory  of  medicine  and  anatomy 
at  Rome,  put  emphasis  on  the  fibers  of  the  muscles 
and  the  nerves,  and  so  foreshadowed  the  importance 
that  was  to  be  given  in  the  eighteenth  century  to  the 
intrinsic  structural  properties  of  these  tissues.  He  de- 

39.  Robinson,  Br\an  (1680- 1754).  A  treatise  of  the  Animal 
Oeconomy  (3rd  ed.).  London:  Innys,   1738. 

40.  Stahl,  Georg  Ernst  (1660-1734).  Theoria  Medica  Vera 
Physiologiam  et  Pat/iologium,  tanquam  Doctrinae  Medicae 
Partes  veres  Conternplativas  e  Naturae  et  Artis  veris  Junda- 
mentis.  Halle,  1708. 

41.  BosTOCK,  John  (1773-1846).  Sketch  of  the  History  of  Medi- 
cine from  Its  origin  to  the  commencement  of  the  nineteenth  century. 
London:  Sherwood,  Gilbert  &  Piper,  1835. 

42.  Vicq  d'.'Kzvr,  F.  (1748-1794).  Oeuvres  de  Vicq  d'Azyr. 
Paris,  1805,  vol.  4. 


FIG.  4.  (iiorgio  Bagli\i  rising  like  a  phoenix  from  the  flames. 


veloped  a  theory  (43)  of  an  oscillatory  movement  of 
nerve  fibers  in  order  to  account  for  both  efferent  and 
afferent  activity  and  envisaged  the  dura  mater  as  the 
source  of  these  movements  and  the  recipient  of  the 
returning  oscillation. 

The  leading  medical  center  in  Europe  at  this 
time  was  the  University  of  Leiden.  The  empirical 
approach  was  urged  by  the  physicist  S'Gravesande 
(44)  who  advised  that  "It  is  Nature  herself  that 
should  be  examined  as  closely  as  possible  .  .  .  progress 
may  be  slow,  but  what  we  find  will  be  certain." 
Petrus  van  Mmschenbroek  (45),  who  had  come  to 
the  Chair  of  Physics  at  Leiden  from  Utrecht  in  1 740, 
had  in  a  discourse  on  scientific  method  emphasized 
that  physics  should  stand  apart  from  metaphysics, 
that  experimental  analysis  should  antecede  synthesis, 
that  in  the  collection  of  evidence  the  exception  should 
not  be  ignored,  and  that  argument  by  analogy  was 
fraught  with  danger.  Yet  it  was  essentially  by  analogy 
that   the  early  eighteenth  century  viewed  the  func- 

43.  Baglivi,  Giorgio  (1668-1707).  De  fibra  motrice  et 
morbosa.  In:  Opera  Omnia.  Leyden:  Antonii  Servant,  1733. 

44.  S'Gravesande,  Wilhelm  Jacob  (1688-1742).  Physices 
Elementa  Mathematica  Experimenlis  conjirmata  sire  Intro- 
ductio  ad  Philosopham  .\ewtoniatinm.  2nd  ed.,  1725;  3rd 
ed.,  1742,2  vols.  Leiden. 

45.  VAN  MusscHENBROEK,  Petrus  (1692-1761).  Discours  a 
i' Organisation  de  V  Experience.  1730.  (His  swansong  as 
Rector  at  the  University  of  Utrecht.) 


10 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSIOLOGV  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGS'    I 


FIG.  5.  Bocrhaave  giving  a  class  in  botany.  (From  the  en- 
graving by  Jacob  Folkema,  reproduced  by  permission  of  the 
Rijksuniversiteit  in  Leiden.) 


tions  of  the  nervous  system;  the  brain  was  analogous 
to  the  heart  and  the  nerves  analogous  to  the  arteries. 
In  the  one  case  the  content  was  blood;  in  the  other, 
nervous  fluid.  Some  writers  even  spoke  of  "the  systole 
of  the  brain  .  .  .  whereby  the  animal  Juices  are  forci- 
bly driven  into  Fibres  of  the  Nerves"  (46). 

van  Musschenbroek  had  been  a  pupil  of  Hermanii 
Boerhaave  who  came  to  the  Chair  of  Medicine  in 
Leiden  in  1701.  Boerhaave,  essentially  a  chemist 
and  a  clinician,  had  an  almost  leaiendary  fame  as  a 
teacher,  which  must,  one  feels,  have  been  due  to  his 
personality,  for  he  was  not  an  experimenter  and  his 
doctrines  were  not  at  all  progressive.  He  added  little 
if  anything  new  to  the  existing  body  of  physiological 

46.   Robinson,  Nichol.a.s.  A  new  system  of  /he  Spleen,   Vapours, 
and  Hypoehondriak  Melanchoh.  London,    1729,  p.  '^62. 


knowledge.  In  his  lectures  (47,  48)  on  the  nervous 
system  he  taught  that  "The  Ventricles  of  the  Brain 
have  also  many  Uses  or  Ad\antages  in  Life,  such  as 
the  perpetual  Exhalation  of  a  thin  \'apour,  or  moist 
Dew."  Himself  a  chemist,  he  made  no  experiments 
in  ph\siology  and  was  content  to  teach  that  "Tho' 
the  nervous  Juice  or  Spirits  separated  in  the  Brain 
are  the  most  subtile  and  moveable  of  any  Humour 
throughout  the  whole  Body,  yet  are  they  formed 
like  the  rest  from  the  same  thicker  Fluid  the  Blood, 
passing  thro'  many  Degrees  of  Attenuation,  till  its 
Parts  become  small  enough  to  pervade  the  last 
Series  of  Vessels  in  the  Cortex,  and  then  it  becomes 
the  subtile  Fluid  of  the  Brain  and  Nerves."  His  au- 
thority for  this  doctrine  which  he  handed  on  to  his 
eighteenth  century  pupils  was  the  works  of  Galen 
who  had  died  in  200  A.D.  These  teachings  are  difficult 
to  reconcile  with  the  exhortation  expressed  in  his 
Aphorismi  (49)  that  attention  to  facts  and  observations 
is  the  best  means  of  promoting  medical  knowledge. 

Yet  among  his  pupils  Boerhaave  numbered  nearly 
all  the  prominent  students  of  the  nervous  system  in 
the  eighteenth  century:  Haller,  van  Swieten,  Monro, 
CuUen,  de  Haen,  Pringle.  His  pre-eminence  lay 
in  the  clinical  field,  and  there  can  be  no  doubt  that 
he  had  the  greatest  gift  of  a  teacher,  that  of  lighting 
the  fire  of  enthusiasm  in  his  students.  It  was  two  of 
them,  Haller  (50)  and  \an  Swieten  (51),  who  were 
responsible  for  the  wider  publication  of  his  lectures, 
for  on  his  own  initiative  he  published  little. 

van  Swieten,  who  as  a  Catholic  had  little  chance  of 
advancement  at  the  L'niversity  of  Leiden,  went  to 
Austria  under  the  patronage  of  Maria  Theresa  and 
there  founded  the  'Old  Vienna  School,'  patterning  it 
on  the  medical  clinic  at  Leiden.  He  was  an  advocate 
of  a  spare  diet  and  active  exertion  and  quoted  in  sup- 
port of  his  views  "the  case  of  a  rich  priest,  who  had 

47.  BoERH.^.WE,  Herm.-^nn  (1668-1738).  Instituliones  Medicae 
in  usus  animal  exercitationis  domesticae.  Leyden,  1708;  anony- 
mous English  translation.  Academical  Lectures  on  the  Theory 
of  Physic,  being  a  genuine  translation  of  his  Institutes,  and  Ex- 
planatory Comment.  London:  Innys,  1743.  5  v'ol. 

48.  BoERH.^.AVE,  H.  Praelectiones  Academicae  de  .\lorbis  Nervorum. 
Quas  ex  Audilorwn  Manuscriptis  collectas  edi  curavil.  Jacobus 
van  Eems.  Leyden:  van  der  Eyk  &  Pecker,   1761.  2  vol. 

49.  BoERH.^AVE,  H.  .Aphorismi  de  cognoscendis  et  curandis 
morbis.  Leyden:  van  der  Linden,  1709. 

50.  VON  Haller,  Albrecht  (i 708-1 777).  Commentarii  ad 
Hermann  Boerhaave  Praelectiones  Academicae  in  proprias 
Irutitutiones  Rei  Medicae.  1 739-1 744.  7  vol. 

51.  VAN  Swieten,  Gerhard  L.B.  (1700-1772).  Commentaria 
in  Hermanni  Boerhaave,  aphorismos,  de  cognoscendis  et  curandis 
morbis.  Leiden:  Verbeek,    1742-1776.  6  vol. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY  I  I 


FIG.  6.  Albrecht  von  Haller,  the  greatest  physiologist  of  the  eighteenth  century,  and  de  La  Mettric 
whose  treatise  L'homme  machine,  addressed  to  Haller,  caused  a  controversy  that  highlighted  the  ques- 
tion as  to  whether  the  soul  lay  in  the  province  of  the  physiologist.  The  portrait  of  Haller  is  from  the 
frontispiece  of  his  Elementa  Physiologiae  and  is  an  engraving  by  Tardieu;  that  of  de  La  Mettrie  is 
from  an  engraving  in  the  Bibliotheque  Nationale  (reproduced  here  with  permission),  the  original 
painting  being  a  pastel  by  Maurice  Quentin  La  Tour. 


enjoyed  a  fat  living  and  long  been  a  martyr  to  gout, 
chancing  to  be  carried  into  slavery  by  a  Barbary 
corsair,  and  kept  for  two  years  to  hard  labour  and 
spare  diet  in  the  gallics  lost  his  gout  and  his  obesity 
together.  ..."  His  master,  Boerhaave,  a  martyr  to 
gout,  had  died  34  years  before,  corpulence  hastening 
his  end. 

We  have  a  contemporary  description  (52)  of  Boer- 
haave's  habits  and  also  of  his  looks.  "He  had  a  large 
head,  short  neck,  florid  complexion,  light  brown  hair 
(for  he  did  not  wear  a  wig),  and  open  countenance, 
and  resembled  Socrates  in  the  flatness  of  his  nose.  ..." 
We  are  told  that  he  ro.se  at  four  in  the  inorning,  but 
in  the  cold  Dutch  winters  he  allowed  himself  an  e.xtra 
hour  in  bed  before  settling  to  work  in  his  unhealed 
study.  His  chief  relaxation  was  music  and  he  played 
several  instruments  of  which  his  favorite  was  the  lute. 

It  is  at  about  this  period — the  middle  of  the  eight- 
eenth century — that  experimental  work  on  the  nerv- 
ous system  began  to  be  channeled  into  three  main 
divisions:    a)    the    elucidation    of    peripheral    nerve 

52.  Burton,  William.  An  account  of  the  Life  and  Writings  of 
Hermann  Boerhaave.   London:  Lintot,    1743. 


physiology  and  its  differentiation  from  that  of  muscle, 
A)  the  recognition  of  the  function  of  the  spinal  cord 
together  with  the  development  of  ideas  about  reflex 
action,  and  c)  the  growth  of  knowledge  about  the 
brain  as  a  neural  structure  unencumbered  by  dogma 
concerning  the  soul. 


EXCIT.iiiBILITY    .AND    TR.ANSMISSION    IN    NERVES 

In  the  field  of  physiology  Boerhaave's  most  prom- 
inent pupil  was  Albrecht  von  Haller.  Haller,  a  Swiss, 
was  born  in  Berne  and  studied  at  Tubingen  but  was 
drawn  to  Leiden  by  the  magnet  of  Boerhaave's 
teaching.  After  taking  his  medical  degree  he  returned 
to  .Switzerland  where  he  divided  his  time  between 
medicine,  poetry  and  botany.  In  1736  George  II  of 
England,  Elector  of  Hanover,  appointed  him  to  the 
chair  of  the  mixed  sciences  Anatomy,  Surgery  and 
Botany  at  Gottingen,  a  newly-founded  university. 
It  was  here  that  Haller  spent  the  experimental  phase 
of  his  life  as  a  scientist. 

Unlike  his  master  Boerhaave,  Haller  was  a  great 
laboratory  worker  as  well  as  a  phenomenal  scholar 


12  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7.  Two  men  whose  ideas  of  irritability  anteceded  tiiose  of  Haller.  Glisson's  concept  (1677) 
included  a  psychic  stage  between  stimulus  and  contraction  thereby  differing  from  Haller's  which 
postulated  a  purely  peripheral  reaction.  Johannes  de  Gorters  proposal  of  irritability  based  on 
mechanical  movement  was  published  in  1734.  The  portrait  of  Glisson  is  an  engraving  from  the 
original  painting  in  the  Royal  College  of  Physicians.  That  of  de  Gorter  is  photographed  from  an 
engraving,  the  generous  gift  of  the  Director  of  the  National  Museum  of  Science  in  I^eiden.  The 
original  painting  was  by  J.  M.  Quinkhard,  the  artist  of  the  portrait  of  van  Musschenbroek  repro- 
duced in  figure  1 1 . 


and  was  the  author  of  the  most  famous  of  the  eight- 
eenth century  textbooks  of  physiology,  the  Elementa 
Physiologiae  (53).  Although  these  volumes  came  into 
print  after  Haller's  retirement  to  Berne,  he  had  while 
teaching  at  Gottingen  brought  out  his  Primae  Lineae 
Physiologiae  (54)  for,  as  he  proceeded  with  his  ana- 
tomical and  experimental  studies,  his  master's  texts 
became  less  and  less  useful  to  him.  In  the  preface  to 
his  own  work  he  remarks  that,  since  the  time  of  Boer- 
haave,  anatomy  had  developed  so  greatly  as  to  be- 
come almost  a  new  science.  Haller  had  himself 
brought  out  an  anatomy  book  (55)  with  fine  engrav- 
ings, and  anatomy  was  one  of  the  four  subjects  on 
which  he  compiled  bibliographies  (56-59)  that  are  a 


great  source  of  information  for  the  medical  historian. 
They  contain  tens  of  thousands  of  references. 

For  neurophysiologists  Haller's  most  interesting 
work  is  his  development  of  the  concept  of  irritability. 
An  earlier  student  of  Boerhaave's  at  Leiden  was 
Johannes  de  Gorter  who  later  became  physician  to 
the  Empress  Elizabeth  of  Russia.  He  had  in  1737 
published  a  volume  (60)  in  which  he  brought  out  of 
obscurity  the  idea  of  the  intrinsic  irritability  of  tissues 
that  had  been  postulated  by  Francis  Glisson  in  the 
previous  century.  It  is  not  clear  whether  de  Gorter 
owed  any  of  his  ideas  to  Glisson.  He  mentions  hiin 
only  once  (in  De  AIolii  vitale,  paragraph  58,  p.  40)  and 
this  only  in  reference  to  the  capsula  hepatis.  In  any 


53.  VON  Haller,  Albrecht  (i  708-1 777).  Elementa  Physi- 
ologiae corporis  humani.  Lausanne:  Marci-Michael  Bous- 
quet  et  Soc,  1 757-1 765.  8  \ol. 

54.  VON  H.'iLLER,  A.  Primae  lineae  physiologiae  in  usiim  praelec- 
tionium  academicarium.  Gottingen:  Vandenhoeck,   1747. 

55.  VON  Haller,  A.  hones  analomicae.  Gottingen:  Vanden- 
hoeck, 1 743-1 756. 

56.  VON  Haller,  A.  Bibliotheca  Bolanica.  Zurich:  Orell,  1771- 
1772. 


57.  VON  Haller,  A.  Bibliotheca  Chirurgica.  Basle:  Schweig- 
hauser,   1774;  Berne:  E.  Haller,   1775. 

58.  VON    Haller,    A.    Bibliotheca    Anatomica.    Zurich:    Orell, 

I774-I777- 

59.  VON  Haller,  A.  Bibliotheca  medicinae  practicae.  Basle: 
Schweighauscr,   1776;  Berne:  E.  Haller,   1778. 

60.  DE  Gorter,  Johannes  (1689-1762).  Exercitaliones  medicae 
quatuor.  I:  De  motu  vitale,  1734;  il:  Somno  et  vigilia; 
HI:  De  Jame;  IV:  De  Siti.  Amsterdam,  1737. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


13 


FIG.  8.  Swammerdam's  experiments  including  the  one  by  which  he  proved  that  muscles  were  not 
swollen  by  an  influx  of  nervous  fluid  when  they  contracted.  Fig.  V  is  of  an  experiment  to  show  the 
change  in  shape  of  a  muscle  when  stimulated  by  pinching  its  nerve.  Fig.  VI  illustrates  the  pulling 
together  of  the  pins  holding  the  tendons  when  the  muscle  contracts.  Fig.  VIII  is  the  crucial  one  in 
which  a  drop  of  water  is  imprisoned  in  the  narrow  tube  projecting  from  the  vessel  enclosing  the 
muscle.  Swammerdam  found  that  when  he  stimulated  the  nerve  by  pulling  it  down  by  a  wire,  the 
muscle  contracted  but  the  drop  of  water  did  not  move.  He  concluded  that  the  volume  of  the  muscle 
did  not  expand  on  contraction.  It  is  the  fact  that  the  wire  was  made  of  silver  (filium  argenteum) 
and  the  loop  of  copper  (filium  aeneum)  that  has  credited  Swammerdam  with  the  use  of  bimetallic 
electricity  as  a  stimulus  to  nerve.  Some  authors  however  interpret  the  action  in  this  experiment  as 
the  mechanical  pull  on  the  nerve.  Some  originals  of  Swammerdam's  plates  can  be  seen  at  the 
National  Museum  of  the  History  of  Science  in  Leiden.  (From  Biblia  .Naturae.  Amsterdam,  1 738). 


case  his  concept  of  intrinsic  irritability  differed  from 
that  of  GHsson  in  being  part  of  a  dynamic  scheme  in 
which  inovements  of  muscles  and  nerves  acted  me- 
chanically on  each  other  (61).  Glisson  (62)  had  been 
among  the  few  scientists  of  the  seventeenth  century  to 
test  experimentally  the  Galenist  doctrine  that  muscu- 
lar contraction  was  due  to  an  inflow  of  nervous  fluid 
inflating  the  muscle.  He  had  demonstrated  by  immer- 
sion of  a  inan's  arm  in  water  that  the  level  did  not 
rise  on  contraction.  Swammerdam,'"  in  Holland, 
reached  the  same  conclusion  from  experiments  on 
frogs  (fig.  8).  From  such  experiments,  Glisson  had 
gone  on  to  develop  a  concept  of  intrinsic  irritability 
varying  in  kind  for  the  different  nervous  functions. 
As  Regius  Professor  of  Physic  at  Cambridge,  Glisson 

61.  DE  GoRTER,  J.  Exercilaliones   Medico  Qiiinta  V:  De   aclione 
viventium  particulari.  Amsterdam,  1 748. 

62.  Glisson,   Francis    (1597^1677).    Traciatus   de   venlricuto  el 
inleslinis.  London:  Henry  Brome,   1677. 


was  to  a  certain  extent  bound  by  the  statutes  goxern- 
ing  these  professorships  to  teach  the  doctrines  of 
Hippocrates  and  of  Galen,  and  this  may  have  limited 
him  in  the  development  of  this  new  idea  of  irrita- 
bility. 

In  Haller's  hands  the  idea  blossomed  into  a  concept 
that  was  to  dominate  physiology  for  over  a  century. 
His  theory  differed  from  Glisson's  in  that  he  omitted 
the  intermediate  element  of  psychic  perception  be- 
tween the  irritation  and  the  contraction.  The  first 
expression  of  his  theory  of  the  relationship  of  con- 
tractility to  irritability  is  found  in  1 739  in  his  com- 
mentaries on  Boerhaave's  lectures  and  a  fuller  de- 
velopment in  his  Elementa  Physiologiae,  but  it  is  in  his 

"No  known  portrait  of  Swammerdam  exists.  In  the  nine- 
teenth century  a  publisher  took  one  of  the  heads  from  Rem- 
brandt's Anatomy  Lesson  and  put  out  a  lithograph  w'nich  he 
labelled  with  Swammerdam's  name.  This  was  a  stroke  of 
imagination  rather  than  fact. 


H 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Gottingen  lectures  (63)  gi\cn  in  1752  (and  published 
the  following  year)  that  the  concept  is  most  full\-  de- 
veloped and  supported  by  experimentation.  Haller's 
own  definitions  for  the  dual  properties  of  irritability 
and  sensibility  were  as  follows:  "I  call  that  part  of  the 
human  body  irritable,  which  becomes  shorter  on 
being  touched;  very  irritable  if  it  contracts  upon 
slight  touch,  and  the  contrary  if  by  a  violent  touch  it 
contracts  but  little.  I  call  that  a  sensible  part  of  the 
human  body,  which  on  being  touched  transmits  the 
impression  of  it  to  the  soul;  and  in  brutes,  in  whom  the 
existence  of  a  soul  is  not  so  clear,  I  call  those  parts 
sensible,  the  Irritation  of  which  occasions  evident 
signs  of  pain  and  disquiet  in  the  animal." 

One  sees  immediately  the  bogey  of  the  early  physi- 
ologists raising  its  head — the  necessity,  on  invoking 
the  soul,  for  differentiating  processes  in  man  from 
those  in  animals.  Haller  describes  his  technique  for 
determining  sensibility  as  follows:  "I  took  living  ani- 
mals of  different  kind,  and  different  ages,  and  after 
laying  bare  that  part  which  I  wanted  to  examine,  I 
waited  till  the  animal  ceased  to  struggle  or  complain, 
after  which  I  irritated  the  part,  by  blowing,  heat, 
spirit  of  wine,  the  scalpel,  lapis  infinalis,  oil  of  vine- 
gar, and  bitter  antimony.  I  examined  attentively, 
whether  upon  touching,  cutting,  burning,  or  lacerat- 
ing the  part,  the  animal  seemed  disquieted,  made  a 
noise,  struggled,  or  pulled  back  the  wounded  limb, 
if  the  part  was  convulsed,  or  if  nothing  of  all  this 
happened." 

Haller  recognized  that  nerves  arc  "the  source  of  all 
sensibility,"  but  applied  his  dichotomy  of  irritability 
and  sensibility  to  various  types  of  nerves,  noting  that 
all  nerves  are  not  irritable  according  to  his  definition 
(with  its  insistence  on  resultant  contraction).  He  thus 
approached  the  differentiation  of  motor  and  sensory 
nerves.  Still  incorporated  in  his  hypothesis  was  the 
1600-year-old  concept  of  a  nervous  fluid  within  the 
nerves.  It  might  be  thought  that  once  the  microscope 
had  been  invented,  the  question  of  whether  or  not 
the  nerves  were  hollow  pipes  might  have  been 
quickly  settled.  Indeed  in  1674  Leeuwenhoek  (64), 
with  the  limited  magnification  of  his  simple  micro- 

63.  VON  Haller,  A.  De  paitibus  corporis  humani  scnsibilibus 
et  irritabilibus.  Comment.  Soc.  reg.  Set.  Gottingen  2:  114, 
1753;  English  translation  by  M.  Tissot,  M.D.  A  disserta- 
tion on  the  sensible  and  irritable  parts  oj  animals^  from  a 
treatise  published  in  the  Transactions  of  the  Royal  Society 
of  Gottingen  and  read  in  the  .'\cademy  of  Gottingen  by 
Haller  on  April  22,  1752.  Printed  by  J.  Nourse  at  the 
Lamb  opposite  Katherine-street  in  the  Strand,  1755. 

64.  VAN  Leeuwenhoek,  .■\ntonj  (1632-1723).  Phil.  Trans.  9: 
178,  1674. 


scope,  had  specifically  searched  for  cavities  in  the 
nerves  of  a  cow  but  his  results  were  equivocal.  One 
hundred  years  later  this  issue  was  still  unresolved. 

The  only  competing  hypothesis,  which  received 
but  little  support,  was  that  the  nerves  were  cords 
that  communicated  sensation  to  the  brain  by  their 
\ibrations  (rejected  by  Boerhaave  as  "repugnant  to 
the  Nature  of  the  soft,  pulpy  and  flaccid  nerves"). 
This  view  was  also  rejected  by  Haller. 

In  considering  how  a  fluid  could  possibly  flow  as 
swiftly  as  nerves  can  be  observed  to  act,  Haller  pro- 
posed that  it  must  indeed  be  a  very  subtle  fluid  imper- 
ceptible to  the  eye  yet  more  substantial  than  heat, 
aether,  electricity  or  magnetism.  In  another  comment 
he  granted  that  electricity  was  a  most  powerful  stimu- 
lus to  nerves  but  that  he  thought  it  improbable  that 
the  natural  stimulus  was  electrical.  Thinking  always 
in  terms  of  electricity  flowing  as  down  a  wire,  Haller, 
like  so  many  physiologists  after  him,  felt  the  lack  of 
insulation  around  the  nerve  to  be  a  critical  argument 
against  nervous  influence  being  electrical. 

However,  the  notion  of  electricity  as  a  transmitter 
of  nervous  acti\ity  kept  cropping  up  at  about  this 
time.  Alexander  Monro  (65),  Professor  of  Medicine 
and  Anatomy  in  the  University  of  Edinburgh,  a 
pupil  of  Boerhaave  and  first  of  the  great  dynasty  of 
Monros,  pointed  out  that  no  cavities  could  be  seen  in 
nerves,  that  no  drops  of  fluid  came  out  when  a  nerve 
was  cut,  and  that  the  nerve  did  not  swell  when  ligated; 
and  he  rather  cautiously  skirted  the  possibility  of 
electricity  being  the  agent.  But  he  too  considered  it 
only  in  terms  of  electricity  running  down  a  wire  and, 
like  Haller,  was  bothered  that  the  nerve  was  inade- 
quately insulated  to  prevent  loss.  "We  are  not  suffi- 
ciently acquainted,"  he  said,  "with  the  properties  of 
aether  or  electrical  effluvia  pervading  everything,  to 
apply  them  justly  in  the  aniitial  oeconomy;  and  it  is 
difficult  to  conceive  how  they  should  be  retained  or 
conducted  in  a  long  nervous  cord." 

Electricity  had  also  been  suggested  by  Stephen 
Hales  (66)  in  refuting  a  suggestion  that  the  swelling 
of  muscles  was  due  to  inflow  of  blood.  This  country 
clergyman,  without  formal  scientific  or  medical  train- 
ing, by  his  experimental  skill  and  keen  observation 
became  one  of  the  outstanding  contributors  to  knowl- 
edge of  the  circulation.  In  writing  of  the  nerves  he 
said,  "From  this  very  small  Force  of  the  arterial  Blood 

65.  Monro,  .-Xlexander  (1697-1762).  The  works  of  Alexander 
Monro  (collected  by  his  son).  Edinburgh:  Charles  Eliot, 
1781. 

66.  Hales,  Stephen  (1677-1761).  Statical  Essays.  London: 
Innys  and  Manby,  vol.  I,   1726;  vol.  II,   1732. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHVSIOLOG V  I  5 


^f^'l 


FIG.  9.  Thf  Abbe  Nollt-t  and  some  of  his  experiments  in  which  he  electrified  plants  and  animals. 
The  portrait,  which  shows  the  Abbe  in  his  study  at  La  Mouette,  is  from  the  oil  painting  by  Jacques 
de  Lajoue  that  hangs  in  the  Musee  Carnavalet  in  Paris  and  is  reproduced  by  kind  permission  of  the 
Conservateur,  M.  Charageat.  The  illustration  on  the  right  is  from  Nollet's  book,  Recherches  sur  les 
Causes  Particuliers  des  Phenomhies  Electnques.  Paris,  I  749. 


among  the  muscular  Fibres  we  may  with  good  reason 
conclude,  how  short  this  Force  is  of  producing  so 
great  an  Effect,  as  that  of  muscular  Motion,  which 
wonderful  and  hitherto  inexplicable  Mystery  of  Na- 
ture, must  therefore  be  owing  to  some  more  vigorous 
and  active  Energy,  whose  Force  is  regulated  by  the 
Nerves;  but  whether  it  he  confined  in  Canals  within 
the  Nerves,  or  acts  along  their  surfaces  like  electrical 
Powers,  is  not  easy  to  determine." 

At  the  end  of  the  century  came  Galvani.  His 
famous  Commentary,  published  first  in  1791,  appeared 
at  a  time  of  intense  interest  in  electricity.  The  demon- 
stration by  Stephen  Gray  (67)  in  England  that  the 
human  body  could  be  electrified  had  been  taken  up 
and    popularized    by   the   Abbe   Nollet   (68)   at    the 


French  Court  and  by  Hausen  (69),  the  Professor  of 
Mathematics  in  Leipzig.  Each  had  copied  Gray's  ex- 
periment in  which  he  suspended  a  boy  by  ropes  from 
the  ceiling,  bringing  a  flint-glass  tube  that  had  been 
charged  by  friction  close  to  his  feet  and  watching  the 
attraction  of  a  leaf-brass  electroscope  to  his  nose  (see 
fig.  10). 

Electroscopes  of  this  primitixe  type  were  the  only 
instruments  then  available  for  the  detection  of  elec- 
tricity, the  most  sensitive  one  being  that  developed  by 
the  curate  of  a  rural  parish  in  Derbyshire  (70).  This 
delicate  instrument  with  its  gold  leaves  was  identified 
by  his  name  as  Bennet's  electrometer,  though  it  was 
.scarcely  a  metrical  device.  Sources  of  electricity  were 
still  the  frictional  machines,  first  globes  of  sulphur, 
gla.ss  or  porcelain,  and  later  revoking  discs.   It  was 


67.  Gray,  Stephen  (?-i736).  Experiments  concerning  elec- 
tricity. Phil.  Trans.  37:  18,  1731. 

68  Noi,i.ET,  Jean-Antoine  (1700-1770).  Essai  sur  I'electricile 
des  corps.  Paris:  Guerin,  1746. 


69.  Hausen,  Christian  August  (1693-1743).  Novi  projeclus  in 
historia  electricitatis.  Leipzig,  I743' 

70.  Bennet,  Abraham  (1750- 1799).  .New  Experiments  on  Elec- 
tricity. Derby  :  John  Drewry,    I  789. 


1 6  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  lo.  The  experiment  of  electrifying  a  boy,  from  the  French  translation  of  the  book  by  F.  H. 
VVinckler  (Professor  of  Greek  and  Latin  at  Leipzig)  entitled,  Essai  sur  la  Nature.  Les  ejjets  el  les  causes 
avec  description  de  deux  nouvelles  machines  a  Eleclricite.  Paris:  Jorry,  1748.  (Photographed  from  the 
copy  in  the  Wheatland  collection  by  kind  permission  of  Mr.  David  Wheatland.) 


vw,.  I  I.  \  an  Mnsschciiljroek  and  a  Leyden  jar.  The  portrait 
is  from  the  oil  painting  by  J.  M.  Quinkhard  which  hangs  in  the 
Museum  of  the  History  of  Science  in  Leiden.  The  jar  is  an 
early  one,  rather  large  in  size,  also  from  the  same  museum, 
by  the  courtesy  of  which  these  photographs  are  reproduced. 


not  until  the  development  of  the  Leyden  jar  by  Petrus 
van  Musschenbroek,  Professor  of  Physics  in  Leiden, 
that  physiologists  gained  a  much  more  stable  and 
powerful  source  of  electricity. 


van  Musschenbroek,  striving  to  conserve  elec- 
tricity in  a  conductor  and  to  delay  the  loss  of  its 
charge  in  air,  attempted  to  use  water  as  the  con- 
ductor, insulating  it  from  air  in  a  nonconducting  glass 
jar.  However,  when  he  charged  the  water  through  a 
wire  leading  from  an  electrical  machine,  he  found  the 
electricity  dissipated  as  quickly  as  e\er.  His  assistant, 
Andreas  Cuneus,  while  holding  a  jar  containing 
charged  water,  accidentally  touched  the  inserted  wire 
with  his  other  hand  and  got  a  frightening  shock.  W'ith 
one  hand  he  had  formed  one  'plate,'  the  charged 
water  being  the  other,  and  the  glass  jar  the  inter- 
\ening  dielectric.  A  condenser  was  born.  On  touching 
the  wire  with  his  other  hand  he  had  shorted  this 
condenser  through  his  body  giving  himself  such  a 
jolt  that  he  thought  "his  end  had  come"  Q]i).  van 
Musschenbroek  wrote  to  Reamur  describing  a  similar 
experience.  Storage  of  electricity  had  now  become 
pos.sible  and  in  fact  had  been  achieved  independently 
by  almost  the  same  means  (an  electrified  nail  dipping 
into  a  vial  containing  liquid)  by  von  Kleist  (72)  of 

71.  Quoted  in  J. -A.  Nollet.  Metnoire  de  l' Academic  Royale  de 
Sciences.  Paris,  1746,  p.  1-25. 

72.  VON  Kleist,  Ewald  Juroen  (d.  1748).  Letter  to  J.  G. 
Kriigcr,  quoted  in  Geschichte  der  Erde  Halle  1746,  p.  177; 
and  letter  to  Winkler  (J.  H.  Winkler.  Die  Eigenschaften 
der  electrischen  Materie  und  des  electrischen  Feuers.  Leipzig, 
■  745)- 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


Kamin  in  Pomerania,  yet  another  of  the  indefatigable 
company  of  eighteenth  century  clergymen  to  whom 
science  owes  so  much. 

Both  the  electroscope  and  the  Lcsclen  jar  were 
used  by  Galvani  in  the  experiments  he  had  begun 
not  later  than  1 780.  He  was  also  familiar  with  the 
fact  that  some  animal  forms,  notably  the  marine  tor- 
pedo and  the  electric  eel,  had  intrinsic  electricity. 
Scientific  studies  of  this  type  of  animal  electricity 
had  begun  with  the  work  of  John  Walsh  (73)  in  1733 
and  have  continued  to  this  day.  In  those  days  the 
production  of  a  spark  was  considered  a  sine  qua  non 
for  full  acceptance  of  the  electrical  nature  of  a 
phenomenon;  this  was  lacking  for  the  fish  until  after 
Gahani's  time  when  Matteucci  developed  a  tech- 
nique for  demonstrating  it  (see  fig.  12).  For  many 
years  before  Galvani's  day,  as  demonstrated  for 
example  by  Swammerdam  and  by  the  French  anato- 
mist Joseph  Guichard  Duverney,^^  it  had  been  known 
that  the  limbs  of  a  frog  could  be  convulsed  by  me- 
chanical irritation,  and  electricity  applied  directly  to 
the  muscle  already  had  been  used  by  many  phy.sicians 
(and  quacks)  to  animate  paralytics. 

The  three  chief  observations  that  stand  out  from 
the  many  experiments  reported  by  Galvani  in  his 
original  Commentarius  (74)  were  a)  that  a  frog's  nerve 
muscle  preparation,  although  at  a  distance  from  a 
sparking  electrostatic  machine,  would  twitch  when 
touched  by  an  observer  (in  the  light  of  later  knowledge 
this  was  called  induction  at  a  distance,  with  stimula- 
tion occurring  by  the  'returning  stroke'  at  the  moment 
of  sparking);  *)  that  atmospheric  electricity  could  be 
used  to  stimulate  frogs'  legs  if  a  long  wire  were  erected 
(the  principle  of  the  lightning  conductor);  and  c)  that 
frogs'  legs  twitched  when  hung  by  brass  hooks  to  an 
iron  railing  even  in  the  absence  of  a  thunderstorm. 
This  last,  the  most  important  discovery  in  his  first  set 
of  experiments,  was  due  to  the  current  that  flows  be- 
tween dissimilar  metals  when  connected  in  a  circuit, 
though  Galvani  did  not  understand  this  at  the  time 
and  attempted  to  explain  all  his  results  as  the  presence 
of  intrinsic  animal  electricity. 

The  Commentartus  was  reprinted  three  times,  twice 
in  1 791  and  again  in  the  turimlent  year  i  792  (the  year 
that  France  seized  Savoy);  then  it  reached  scientists 

73.  Walsh,  John  (1725-1795).  On  the  electric  property  of 
the  torpedo.  Phil.  Trans.  63:  461,  1773. 

74.  Galvani,  A.  (1737- 1798).  De  viribus  electricitatis  in 
motu  musculari.  Comment ar his  De  Bononiensi  Scientiarum  el 
Artium  Insliluto  alque  Academic  Commenlarii  7:  363,  1 791; 
English  translation  of  2nd  reprinting  of  Galvani's  Com- 
mentary by  M.  G.  Foley.  In:  Galvani:  Effects  of  Electricity 
on  Muscular  Motion.  Norwalk:  Burndy  Library,   1954. 


FIG.  12.  Galvani  and  the  experiment  on  muscle  contraction 
in  the  absence  of  any  metals.  The  portrait  is  from  the  contem- 
porary oil  painting  in  the  Library  of  the  University  of  Bologna 
(reproduced  by  courtesy  of  Dr.  G.  Pupilli).  The  experiment 
in  which  one  leg  is  being  stimulated  by  touching  the  nerves 
from  the  severed  spinal  column  is  reproduced  from  Aldini's 
book,  Essai  sur  Ic  Galvanisme.  Paris:  Piranesi,  1804. 


outside  Italy.  Through  the  great  controversy  stirred 
up  by  Volta  which  continued  after  Galvani's  death  in 
1798  (Galvani's  less  prudent  nephew  Aldini  cham- 
pioning his  cause),  two  extremely  important  areas  of 
knowledge  developed  from  the  original  observations. 
One  was  the  recognition  and  elucidation  of  the 
electrical  properties  of  mu.scle  and  ner\'e  which  were 
to  lead  directly  to  the  discovery  (by  du  Bois-Reymond 
in  the  next  century)  of  the  action  potential  of  nerve, 
and  the  other  was  the  developinent  (by  Volta)  of  bi- 
metallic electricity  into  the  electric  battery,  one  of 
the  major  technological  steps  in  the  history  of  science. 
Volta  had  striven  to  explain  all  the  frog  experiments 
by  bimetallic  currents,  insisting  that  to  produce  elec- 
tricity three  substances  were  always  necessary,  two 
heterogeneous  metals  and  a  third  conducting  material 


"  This,  one  of  the  early  public  demonstrations  of  the  stimula- 
tion of  muscle  through  irritation  of  its  nerve,  was  made  before 
the  Academic  Royalc  de  Sciences  in  Paris  in  1700,  and  is 
reported  for  that  year  as  follows:  "M.  Du  Verney  shewed  a  frog 
just  dead,  which  in  taking  the  nerves  of  the  belly  of  this  animal 
which  go  to  the  thighs  and  legs,  and  irritating  them  a  little 
with  a  scalpel,  trembled  and  suffered  a  sort  of  convulsion. 
Afterwards  he  cut  these  nerves  in  the  belly,  and  holding  them 
a  little  stretched  with  his  hand,  he  made  them  do  so  again  by 
the  same  motion  of  the  scalpel.  If  the  frog  has  been  longer  dead 
this  would  not  have  happened,  in  all  probability  there  yet 
remained  some  liquor  in  these  nerves,  the  undulation  of  which 
caused  the  trembling  of  the  parts  where  they  corresponded, 
and  consequently  the  nerves  are  only  pipes,  the  effect  whereof 
depends  upon  the  liquor  which  they  contain."  History  and 
Memoirs  of  the  Roy.  Acad.  Sci.  Paris.  Translated  and  abridged  by 
John  Martyn  and  Ephraim  Chambers.  London:  Knapton, 
1742, p.  187. 


i8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  13.  Volta  and  the  experiment  cf  Galvani  that  led  to  the  development  of  the  Voltaic  pile. 
The  engraving  of  Volta  is  from  the  drawing  by  Roberto  Focasi.  Volta  was  an  admirer  of  and  was 
honored  by  Napoleon,  one  of  whose  gestures  he  seems  to  have  caught.  Behind  him  is  a  Voltaic  pile. 
The  sketch  at  the  right  was  composed  by  an  artist  from  a  drawing  made  by  du  Bois-Rcymond  when 
he  visited  Galvani's  house  54  years  after  the  latter's  death.  It  depicts  the  experiment  (designed  to 
test  atmospheric  electricity)  in  which  Galvani  stumbled  on  the  phenomenon  of  bimetallic  electricity. 
(From  Reden  von  Emit  du  Bois-Reymond,  1887,  vol.  2.) 


to  complete  the  circuit.  If  thi.s  third  material  were  a 
frog's  muscle,  it  would  by  \irtue  of  its  irritability  react 
to  the  flow  of  bimetallic  electricity,  but  its  role  (ac- 
cording to  Volta)  was  solely  that  of  an  electroscope 
(75).  When  Aldini  (76)  demonstrated  by  dipping 
ends  of  nerve  and  muscle  in  mercury  that  the  same 
effect  could  be  obtained  with  a  single  metal,  Volta 
replied  that  the  .surface  in  contact  with  the  air 
suffered  a  change  that  made  it  heterogeneous  with 
the  depth.  This  tortuous  argument  was  disproved  by 
von  Humboldt  (77). 

Before  Galvani's  death  an  anonymous  (78)  tract 
was  published,  almost  certainly  with  his  collaboration, 
in  which  an  experiment  was  described  on  the  twitch- 
ing of  muscles  in  the  absence  of  any  metals  or  external 

75.  Volta,  Alessandro  (1745-1827).  On  electricity  e.xcited 
by  the  mere  contact  of  conducting  substances  of  different 
kinds.  Phil.  Trans,  go:  403,  1800. 

76.  Aldini,  Giovanni  (1762- 1834).  De  animali  Electricitate 
dissertationes  dime.   Bologna,    1794. 

77.  VON  Humboldt,  Frederick  Alexander  (1769-1859).  Ver- 
suche  iiber  die  gereizle  Muskel-  und  .Nervenjasser.  Posen  und 
Berlin,  1797. 

78.  Anonymous.  Dell'uso  e  dell' attivita  dell'Arco  condultore  nelle 
contrazioni  del  muscoli.  With  Supplemento.  Bologna:  S. 
Tommaso  Aquino,  1 794;  part  of  the  Supplemento  has 
been  translated  into  English  by  M.  Tschou  in:  B.  Dibner. 
Galvani-Volta.  Nor  walk:  Burndy  Library,  1952. 


source  of  electricity.  A  contraction  was  demonstrated 
when  the  cut  end  of  a  frog's  spine  fell  over  onto  its 
muscle  or  when  one  limb  was  drawn  up  to  touch  the 
exposed  sciatic  nerve  (see  fig.  12).  In  this  case  the 
source  of  electricity  was  what  we  now  recognize  as 
the  current  of  injury.  Even  after  this  demonstration 
(79)  Volta  tried  to  explain  the  current  flow  as  the  re- 
sult of  heterogeneity  of  tissues  (muscle  and  nerve). 

The  design  of  Humboldt's  experiments  and  the 
clarity  of  his  reasoning  are  a  pleasure  to  study  in 
the  welter  of  acrimonious  controversy  that  greeted 
GaKani's  findings.  Without  bias  towards  either 
protagonist  Humboldt  repeated  their  experiments, 
examined  their  interpretations,  designed  new  experi- 
ments to  test  their  hypotheses  and  came  to  the  con- 
clusion that  Galvani  uncovered  two  genuine  phe- 
nomena (bimetallic  electricity  and  intrin.sic  animal 
electricity)  and  that  the.se  were  not  mutually  exclu- 
sive. Humboldt  demonstrated  that  both  great  scientists 
erred  in  their  interpretations  of  their  experiments; 
however,  from  these  were  to  grow  the  science  of 
electrophysiology  on  the  one  hand  and,  on  the  other, 
the  brilliant  development  of  the  electric  battery.  Not 
only  does  Humboldt  expose  the  erroneous  parts  of 
Galvani's    and    of   Volta's    interpretations    but    also 

79.   Volta,  .\.  Phil.  Mag.  4:  163,  1799. 


THE    HISTORICAL   DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


those  of  the  writers  who  rushed  in  so  precipitately  to 
take  up  arms  for  one  or  the  other  protagonist — Pfaff 
(80),  Fowler  (81),  Valli  (82),  Schmuck  (83),  each  re- 
ceived his  rebuke.  He  tells  us  that  he  thought  some 
of  the  problems  out  while  sitting  at  the  foot  of  Mt. 
Bernard  reading  de  Saussures'  Voyages  dans  les  Alpes 
(84).  Humboldt  was  a  great  traveller  (especially  at  a 
period  when  he  was  an  inspector  of  mines)  but  did 
not  let  this  interfere  with  his  experiments,  for  he  took 
his  apparatus  along  with  him,  even  on  horseback. 

The  pursuit  of  research  in  animal  electricity  was 
carried  on  in  many  countries,  the  most  valuable  contri- 
butions coming  first  from  the  Italian  .scientists.  Their 
task  was  made  easier  for  them  by  Oersted's  discovery 
of  electromagnetism  and  its  de\elopment  by  Nobili 
into  a  useful  form  of  galvanometer.  Oddly  enough 
Oersted's  researches  (85,  86)  that  led  to  his  important 
experimental  demonstration  of  the  relationship  be- 
tween electricity  and  magnetism  were  motivated  by  a 
metaphy.sical  belief  in  the  universality  of  nature,  a 
faith  inspired  by  his  adherence  to  Natiirphilosophie. 
This  romantic  doctrine  with  its  facade  of  facts  was 
very  powerful  in  Germany  from  about  1810  to  1840 
and  was  derived  from  Kant's  rejection  of  empiricism 
and  his  philosophy  of  universal  laws  known  a  priori 
by  intuition.  Oersted's  own  a  priori  belief  was  so 
strong  that  he  did  not  hesitate  to  make  his  first  experi- 
mental test  of  it  in  the  classroom  during  a  lecture  to 
advanced  students  at  the  University  of  Copenhagen. 
The  experiment  worked;  when  current  flowed  in  a 
single  loop  of  bent  wire,  a  magnet  below  it  moved.  This 
great  discovery  led  to  the  development  of  instruments 
with  multiple  windings  and  to  moving  coil  galvanom- 
eters. The  contribution  of  Nobili,  Professor  of  Physics 

80.  Pfaff,  Chrktophe-Henri  (1773-1858).  ,\bhandlung 
liber  die  sogennante  thierische  Electrizitat.  Gren's  J. 
Physik.  8(2):  196,  1798. 

81.  Fowler,  Richard.  Experiments  and  observations  relative  to 
the  influence  lately  discovered  by  M.  Galvani,  and  (oninionly 
called  Animal  Electricity.  Edinburgh:  Duncan,  1793. 

82.  Valli,  Eusebe  (1755-1816).  Experiments  in  Animal  Elec- 
tricity. London:  Johnson,  1793- 

83.  ScHMiJCK,  Edmund  Joseph.  Beitrdge  zur  neuern  Krnntniss 
der  thierische  Elektricitdt.  Mannheim,   1792. 

84.  DE  Saussures,  H.  B.  Voyages  dans  les  Alpes.  Neuchatel,  1796. 

85.  Oersted,  Hans  Christian  Ci777^i850-  Experiences  sur 
un  effet  que  le  courant  de  la  pile  e.xcite  dans  I'aiguille 
aimantee.  J.  Phys.  Chim.  91:  72,  1820:  English  translation 
in  Ann.  Phil.  16:  273,  1820.  The  earliest  announcement  of 
Oersted's  discovery  was  in  a  four-page  pamphlet  (now 
rare)  entitled  Experimenta  circa  efectum  conflictus  electrici 
in  acum  magneticum.  Copenhagen,  1820  (copy  in  the 
Wheeler  collection,   New  York). 

86.  Oersted,  H.  C.  Galvanic  magnetism.  Phil.  .\iag.  56:  394, 
1820. 


FIG.  14.  Matteucci  and  two  of  his  experimental  procedures. 
The  portrait  is  reproduced  from  the  old  yellowing  photograph 
in  the  Schola  Normale  .Superiore  in  Pisa  (by  courtesy  of  Dr.  G. 
Moruzzi).  .-Xbove  on  the  right  is  Malteucci"s  illustration  of  his 
rheoscopic  frog,  and  below  is  his  experiment  demonstrating 
that  the  discharge  of  a  marine  torpedo  can  make  a  spark  cross 
a  gap. 


and  Natural  History  at  Florence,  was  the  astatic 
galvanometer  (87)  in  which  two  coils  of  wire  wound  in 
opposite  directions  cancelled  the  effect  of  the  earth's 
own  magnetism. 

It  was  Matteucci,  the  Professor  of  Physics  at  Pisa, 
who  laid  the  groundwork  of  muscle  electrophysiology 
that  was  to  be  developed  so  exhaustively  by  du  Bois- 
Reymond.  Carlo  Matteucci  (88)  was  one  of  the 
prominent  figures  in  the  Risorgimento.  A  great  liberal 
and  a  great  patriot,  he  attempted  to  coordinate  the 
efforts  of  all  European  liberals  when  the  1 848  revolu- 
tion broke  out.  When  Italy  was  united  in  1859,  he 
was  made  a  Senator.  He  was  one  of  the  early  Min- 
isters for  Public  Instruction  in  Italy.  His  contribu- 
tions have  never  received  adequate  recognition,  mainly 
owing  to  the  acrimonious  attacks  made  on  his  work 
by  du  Bois-Reymond  who  came  near  to  diminishing 
his  own  stature  by  his  sour  polemics.  Matteucci  had 
rai.sed  the  question  as  to  where  in  the  nerve-muscle 

87.  Nobili,  C.  Leopold  (1784-1835).  Uber  einen  neuen 
Galvanometer.  J.  Chem.  u.  Phys.  45:  249,  1825. 

88.  Matteucci,  Carlo  (1811-1865).  Leqons  sur  les  Phe- 
nomenes  Physiques  des  Corps  Vivanls,  translated  by  Clet. 
Paris:  Masson,  1847;  English  translation  by  Jonathan 
Periera.  Lectures  on  the  physical  phenomena  of  living  beings. 
Philadelphia:  Lea  and  Blanchard,   1848. 


20 


HANDBOOK    OF    PPn'SIOI.OGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  15.  Johannes  Miiller  and  his  famous  pupil  von  Hehn- 
holtz.  The  deHcate  chalk  drawing  of  Miiller  was  at  one  time 
in  the  Surgeon  General's  Library  (now  the  National  Library 
of  Medicine).  The  picture  of  von  Helmholtz  shows  him  as  a 
young  man  in  the  period  when  he  made  his  major  contribu- 
tions to  the  physiology  of  peripheral  nerve. 


preparation  the  electricity  lay  and  had  thought  that 
muscle  alone  could  produce  it.  The  preparation 
used  by  Matteucci  was  a  frog's  leg  complete  be- 
low the  knee  with  only  the  isolated  nerve  abo\e 
it.  Galvani's  frogs  retained  a  piece  of  the  vertebral 
column  with  the  insertion  of  the  nerve  into  its 
portion  of  the  spinal  cord.  Matteucci's  contribu- 
tions in  brief  were  a)  the  galvanometric  detection  of 
a  current  flow  between  the  cut  surface  of  a  muscle 
and  its  undamaged  surface,  demonstrated  in  Ijoth 
animal  and  man  (89,  90);  h)  the  multiplication  of 
current  by  serial  arrangement  of  cut  muscles  so  that 
the  transverse  section  of  each  touched  the  longitudinal 
section  of  the  next;  c)  the  decrease  in  this  current  dur- 
ing tetanus  caused  by  strychnine  (90)  (the  germ  of  the 

89.  Matteucci,  C.  Sur  le  courant  electrique  de  la  grenouille. 
Ann.  chim.  et  phys.  68:  93,   1838. 

90.  M.\TTEUcci,  C.  Deu.Kieme  memoire  sur  le  courant  elec- 
trique propre  de  la  grenouille  et  des  animau.x  a  sang 
chaud.  Ann.  chim  et  phys.  80:  301,  1842. 


"  ".  .  .  while  each  organ  of  sense  is  provided  with  a  capacity 
of  receiving  certain  changes  to  be  played  upon  it,  as  it  were, 
yet  each  is  utterly  incapable  of  receiving  the  impression 
destined  for  another  organ  of  sensation."  Quoted  from  Bell, 
Charles  (1774-1842).  Idea  of  a  new  anatomy  of  the  brain,  submitted 
for  the  observation  of  his  friends.  Privately  printed,   1811. 

""It  is  more  probable  that  every  nerve  so  affected  as  to 
communicate  sensation,  in  whatever  psirt  of  the  nerve  the 
impression  is  made,  always  gives  the  same  sensation  as  if 
affected  at  the  common  seat  of  the  sensation  of  that  particular 
nerve.  ..."  Quoted  in  The  Works  of  John  Hunter  edited  by  J.  F. 
Palmer.  London;  Longmans,  1835.  4  vol. 


discovery  of  the  action  current);  and  d)  the  aljility 
of  a  frog's  mu.scle  contraction  to  generate  enough 
electricity  to  stimulate  the  nerve  of  another  nerve- 
muscle  preparation  when  laid  across  it  (the  rheo- 
scopic  frog)  (91,  92).  Matteucci  was  inconsistent  in 
his  interpretation  of  this  finding  and  showed  his 
characteristic  vacillation  between  an  explanation  in 
terms  of  electricity  and  one  based  on  nervous  force. 
He  named  the  effect  the  'secondary  contraction.' 
Matteucci  (93)  also  noted  such  important  laijoratory 
phenomena  as  the  difference  in  stimulating  effect  of 
make'  and  'break'  shock.s,  and  the  polarizing  effects 
of  prolonged  flow  of  current  on  electrodes.  He  noted 
that  polarization  could  occur  inside  the  muscle  and 
thus  laid  the  ground  for  all  the  work  that  was  to 
follow  on  polarization  and  electrotonus. 

du  Bois-Reymond,  of  French  name  and  Swiss 
descent,  lived  all  of  his  working  life  in  Berlin.  He  was 
a  pupil  of  the  greatest  physiologist  of  the  time, 
Johannes  Miiller.  Miiller,  professor  first  at  Bonn  and 
then  at  Berlin,  was  a  gifted  teacher  who  could  count 
among  his  pupils  von  Helmholtz,  von  Briicke  and 
Sechenov.  His  Handbuch  der  Physiologie  (94)  was  the 
great  textbook  of  the  nineteenth  century,  and  the 
journal  he  founded,  Mtiller's  Archives  fiir  Analomie  und 
Physiologie,  as  a  successor  to  Reil's  first  physiological 
journal,  was  the  main  outlet  for  the  stream  of  research 
that  was  coming  from  the  German  schools  at  that  time. 
His  own  interests  lay  mostly  in  sensory  physiology 
where  his  name  is  always  associated  with  the  "Law  of 
.Specific  Nerve  Energies,'  although  this  concept  in 
fragmentary  form  had  certainly  occurred  to  others 
before  him,  including  notably  Charles  Bell''-  and  John 
Hunter.'-'  By  this  law  Miiller  formulated  the  findings 
that  wherever  along  its  course  a  sensory  ner\e  was 
stimulated,  the  resultant  sensation  was  that  appropri- 
ate to  the  sense  organ  it  served.  On  the  issue  of  elec- 
tricity in  nerve,  Miiller  took  the  position  that  it  was 
indeed  an  artificial  excitant  but  had  no  part  in  natural 
excitation.  He  reached  this  conclusion  largely  from  an 
experiment  in  which  he  mashed  the  nerve  and  demon- 

9 1 .  Matteucci,  C.  Sur  une  phenomene  physiologique  produite 
par  les  muscles  en  contraction.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,  sc, 
Paris  4:  797,    1842. 

92.  Matteucci,  C.  and  F.  H.  A.  Humboldt.  Sur  le  courant 
electrique  des  muscles  des  animaux  vivants  ou  recemment 
tues.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,  sc,  Paris  16:  197,  1843. 

93.  M.\TTEUCCi,  C.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,  sc,  Paris  52:  231,  1861 ; 
53:  503,  1861;  56:  760,  1863;  65:  131,   1867. 

94.  MiJLLER,  Johannes  (1801-1858).  Handbuch  der  Physiologic 
des  Menschen.  Coblentz:  Holscher,  vol.  I,  1833;  vol.  II, 
1840;  English  translation  by  William  Baly.  vol.  I,  1838; 
vol.  II,  [842. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


strated  that,  although  electricity  passed  through  the 
damaged  zone,  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  nerve 
above  the  injury  provoked  no  twitch. 

During  the  era  of  intense  concentration  on  electro- 
physiology  in  the  Italian  and  German  schools,  labora- 
tories in  other  countries  were  developing  a  different 
approach.  Among  these  was  that  of  Claude  Bernard 
(95),  pupil  of  Magendie.  Claude  Bernard  made  use  of 
curare  as  a  blocking  agent,  interpreted  by  him  as  a 
nerve  poison  that  spared  the  muscle.  He  found  that 
in  a  curarized  preparation  the  muscle  would  not 
twitch  if  he  stimulated  it  directly  and  hence  concluded 
that  normally  transmission  could  not  be  electrical 
either.  In  these  experiments  he  used  the  ingenious 
little  stimulator  built  from  a  Voltaic  pile  of  alternate 
copper  and  zinc  plates  that  is  shown  in  figure  16. 
He  did  not  recognize  that  his  failure  to  evoke  a  con- 
traction by  direct  stimulation  of  the  muscle  was  due 
to  his  'pile'  giving  too  feeble  a  current. 

Miiller  was  the  last  of  the  great  physiologists  to 
retain  a  trace  of  vitalism  in  his  thinking.  This  he 
probably  owed  to  his  exposure  as  a  student  at  Bonn 
to  Natiirphilosophie  and  the  influence  of  its  leader, 
Schelling  (96).  Although  more  extensively  indoc- 
trinated in  this  sterile  philosophy  than  Oersted  had 
been,  Miiller  was  later  able  to  free  himself  more  ea,sily 
from  its  stultifying  effects,  and  he  eagerly  encouraged 
the  physical  and  chemical  approaches  to  biological 
experiment.  Not  a  trace  of  vitalism  is  found  in  his 
pupils. 

Towards  the  half-century  a  marked  swing  away 
from  the  metaphysics  of  Natiirphilosophie  char- 
acterized neurophysiology,  du  Bois-Rcymond  con- 
sidered himself  (and  with  some  right)  to  be  the 
champion  of  this  movement  which  strove  to  explain 
all  physiology  on  chemical  and  physical  grounds. 
And  in  fact,  as  we  have  .seen,  it  was  the  physicists  of 
the  period  who  were  contributing  most  of  the  new 
experiments  and  concepts  of  muscle  and  peripheral 
nerve  action.  Before  this,  neurophvsiologists  had 
reached  a  stage  in  their  work  in  which  progress  was 
hampered  by  lack  of  sufficiently  sensitive  instruments. 
The  physicists  came  to  their  help  and  indeed  were 
themselves  intrigued  by  the  types  of  physical  phe- 
nomena that  biological  preparations  provided. 

In     1841     du    Bois-Reymond    received    from    his 

95.  Bernard,  Claude  (1813-1878).  Lf(,ons  sur  la  phynoloiie 
el  la pathologie  du  systeme  nerveux.  Paris:  Bailli^re,  1B58.  2  vol. 

96.  Schelling,  Frederick  Wilhelm  Joseph  (i  775-1854). 
Sammiliche  Werke.  .Stuttgart  and  Augsburg,  1856-1861, 
14  vol.;  English  translation  of  vol.  Ill  by  T.  Davidson. 
In  J.  Specula! .  Philos.  I:  193.  1867. 


% 


\        I 


FIG.  16.  Claude  Bernard  at  the  age  of  53,  and  the  ingenious 
stimulators  he  used  in  his  electrophysiological  studies  of  nerve. 
They  were  miniature  voltaic  piles  built  up  of  alternate  discs  of 
copper  and  zinc.  Just  before  use  they  were  moistened  with 
vinegar.  Such  dc\ices  were  made  obsolete  by  the  du  Bois- 
Reymond  induction  coil  and  it  is  rather  surprising  to  find 
Bernard  still  advocating  them  in  his  day.  Although  adequate 
for  nerve  stimulation,  they  gave  too  feeble  a  current  to  stimulate 
a  muscle  directly;  from  this  Bernard  concluded  that  the  nervous 
effect  on  muscle  could  not  be  electrical. 


master  a  copy  of  Matteucci's  book  Essai  sur  les 
Phenomenes  Electriques  des  Animaux  (97),  together  with 
the  suggestion  that  he  repeat  and  extend  Matteucci's 
experiments.  By  November  of  that  year  he  had  al- 
ready completed  a  preliminary  note  C98),  but  his 
major  work,  the  Thierische  Elektricitdt  C99)j  did  not 
appear  until  1848.  The  first  part  of  this  long  and  de- 
tailed book,  unlike  its  later  sections,  shows  little 
originality  in  scientific  ideas,  the  author  with  a  chip 
on  his  shoulder  being  carried  along  in  the  wake  of 
Matteucci  of  whose  publications  he  was  outspokenly 
critical.  However,  where  du  Bois-Reymond  shines, 
and  what  makes  his  book  a  classic,  is  his  skill  in  in- 
struinentation,  far  surpassing  that  of  Matteucci,  so 
that  he  was  able  to  extend  and  improve  on  these 
earlier  observations.  Moreover,  not  being  hampered 
(as  Matteucci  was)  by  residual  traces  of  a  belief  in 

97.  Matteucci,  C.  Essai  sur  les  Phenomenes  electriques  des 
Animaux.  Paris;  Czirilian-Goeury  and  Dalmont,   1840. 

98.  DU  Bois-Revmond,  Emil  (18 18-1896).  Vorlaufiger  Abriss, 
einer  Untersuchung  iiber  den  elektromotorischen  Fische. 
Ann.  Physik.  Chem.  58:  i,  1843. 

99.  DU  Bois-Revmond,  E.  Uniersuchungen  iiber  thierische  Elek- 
tricitdt. Berlin:  Reimer,  vol.  I,  1848;  vol.  II,  1849. 


22 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  17.  du  Bois-Reymond  and  one  of  the  schemata  he  postu- 
lated for  transmission  at  the  end  plate. 


'nerve  force,'  he  brought  clearer  inductive  rea.soning 
to  the  interpretation  of  his  obser\'ations. 

du  Bois-Reymond  confirmed  Matteucci's  demon- 
stration that  not  only  nerve-muscle  preparations  but 
muscles  themselves  could  produce  electricity  and, 
with  .soine  acerbity,  claimed  priority  for  naming  this 
the  'muscular  current'  (Muskelstrom).  Both  Mat- 
teucci  and  du  Bois-Reymond  distinguished  muscular 
current  from  the  frog  current'  (la  correnta  propria 
della  rana),  so  named  by  Nobili  to  describe  the  current 
flow  between  the  feet  of  the  prepared  frog  and  any 
other  part  of  the  animal.  Neither  Noijili  (100)  nor 
Matteucci,  nor  even  du  Bois-Reymond  at  this  time, 
recognized  that  the  so-called  frog  current  was  an 
injury  current  consequent  to  their  having  trans- 
sected  their  frogs.  Nobili  had  thought  it  was  a  thermo- 
electric effect  due  to  differential  cooling  times  of 
nerve  and  muscle. 

du  Bois-Reymond,  using  faradic  stimulation,  also 
confirmed  Matteucci's  finding  that  the  muscle  current 
was  reduced  during  tetanic  stimulation  and  named 
this  the  negative  variation.  It  is  what  is  now  called 
the  action  current  of  muscle,  du  Bois-Reymond  went 
on  to  demonstrate  the  same  negative  variation  in 
nerve  during  activity  and  thus  discovered  the  action 
current  of  nerve  which  Matteucci  had  failed  to  find 
with  his  less  sensitive  instruments,  du  Bois-Reymond 
made  the  following  claim,  "If  I  do  not  greatly  de- 
ceive myself,  I  have  succeeded  in  realizing  in  full  ac- 
tuality (albeit  under  a  slightly  different  aspect)  the 
hundred  years'  dream  of  physicists  and  physiologists, 
to  wit,  the  identity  of  the  nervous  principle  with 
electricity."  His  great  contemporary  Carl  Ludwig 
(loi)  was  unwilling  to  accept  this  for,  thinking  still 
in  terms  of  the  nerve  as  a  telegraph  wire,  he  held 

100.   Nobili,  L.  Ann.  chiin.  el  phys.  38:  225,  1828;  44:  60,  1830. 


(among  other  objections)  that  its  resistance  was  too 
great  and  its  insulation  too  poor  for  it  to  be  a  good 
conductor. 

Pfliiger  (102)  tried  to  o\ercomc  some  of  these 
difficulties  by  his  'liberation  hypothesis.'  In  this  he 
stated  that  nervous  transmission  was  "not  a  simple 
advancing  undulation  in  which  the  sum  of  the  living 
forces  is  not  increased"  but  a  situation  in  which  "new 
tension  forces  are  set  free  by  the  living  forces  of  the 
stimulus  and  become  in  turn  living  forces  with  each 
onward  step."  In  spite  of  the  obscurity  of  the  termi- 
nology (this  is  Morgan's  translation),  one  can  detect  a 
foreshadowing  of  the  ideas  held  by  today's  physi- 
ologists. 

du  Bois-Reymond  elaborated  a  theory  that  all  un- 
damaged muscle  had  a  resting  potential  between  the 
middle  (positive)  and  the  tendons  (negative)  and 
that  during  activity  this  decreased,  thus  giving  the 
'negative  variation.'  He  was  still  not  clear  on  the 
role  of  injury  currents  for  he  thought  injury  merely 
intensified  the  resting  potentials.  On  this  point  he 
entered  into  acrimonious  dispute  with  his  pupil  Her- 
mann who  was  equally  stubborn  in  insisting  that 
there  were  no  resting  potentials  in  the  absence  of  in- 
jury and  that  all  current  flow  in  muscle  and  nerve 
was  due  to  damage  (103).  Hermann  therefore  intro- 
duced the  term  'demarcation  currents'  to  describe 
them.  Later  experimentation  has  shown  both  men  to 
have  been  partially  right  and  partially  wrong. 

du  Bois-Reymond's  conception  of  regularly  oriented 
'electromotive  particles'  arranged  along  the  surface 
of  muscle  and  of  nerve  was  the  forerunner  of  the 
schemata  of  polarization  that  were  to  be  developed 
more  fully  and  more  accurately  by  his  pupil  Berns- 
stein  (104)  and  that  lie  at  the  core  of  modern  theory. 
The  critical  issue  as  to  whether  the  negative  variation 
in  nerve  potential  was  identical  with  the  excitatory 
process  (i.e.  the  nerve  impulse)  was  taken  up  by 
Bernstein  who  set  out,  at  du  Bois-Reymond's  sugges- 
tion, to  compare  their  velocities,  von  Helmholtz,  one 
of  the  same  brilliant  group  schooled  in  Miiller's 
famous  laboratory,  had  in  a  triumph  over  primitive 
apparatus   succeeded   in    measuring    the    velocity    of 

101.  Ludwig,  C.-^rl  (1816-1895).  Uber  die  Ki-afte  der  Ner- 
venprimitivenrohr.   Wien.  med.  Wchnschr.  46:  47,   1861. 

102.  Pfluger,  E.  (1829-1910).  Vnter suchungen  iiber  die  Physi- 
ologie  des  Eleclrolonus.  Berlin:  Hirschwald,  1859. 

103.  Hermann,  Ludim.\r  (i 838-1 91 4).  W'eitere  Untcrsungen 
iiber  die  Ursache  der  electro-motorischen  Erscheinungen 
an  Muskeln  und  Nerven.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  3:15,  1870. 

104.  Bernstein,  Julius  (1839-1 9 17).  Untersuchungen  iiber  den 
Erregungsvorgang  im  Nerien-  und  Muskelsysleme.  Heidelberg  : 
Winter,  1871. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHVSIOLOGV 


23 


the  excitatory  processes  (105)  in  the  frog.  In  his 
success  he  had  proved  his  old  teacher  wrong.  In  1844 
Miiller  had  said,  "The  time  in  which  a  sensation 
passes  from  the  exterior  of  the  brain  and  spinal  cord 
and  thence  back  to  the  muscle  so  as  to  produce  a  con- 
traction, is  infinitely  small  and  immeasurable."  von 
Helmholtz's  technique  was  as  follows:  the  moment  of 
nerve  stimulation,  by  the  break  shock  of  an  induction 
coil,  was  signalled  by  the  closing  of  the  primary  cir- 
cuit. The  resultant  muscle  contraction  lifted  a  contact 
in  the  same  circuit,  thus  breaking  it.  The  break  sig- 
nalled the  arrival  of  the  nerve  impulse  in  the  muscle. 
By  timing  this  inter\al,  with  stimulation  at  measured 
distances  along  the  nerve,  von  Helmholtz  was  able 
to  calculate  its  conduction  velocity.  This  simplified 
description  masks  the  extreme  ingenuity  of  the  original 
experiment.  In  technique  von  Helmholtz  had  coine 
a  long  way  from  Haller's  attempt  to  discover  the 
velocity  of  nervous  action.  Haller  had  read  parts  of 
The  Aeneid  a\oud,  timing  himself,  counting  the  syllables 
and  calculating  the  length  of  the  nervous  paths  used 
in  reading  and  speaking.  In  some  way  that  is  not 
entirely  clear,  he  arrived  at  a  figure  of  50  m  per  sec. 

The  conduction  rate  found  by  Bernstein  (approxi- 
mately 29  m  per  sec.)  tallied  sufficiently  well  with 
von  Helmholtz's  final  results,  27  to  30  m  per  sec,  for 
him  to  be  satisfied  with  the  inferred  identity  of  the 
impulse  and  the  negative  variation.  Bernstein's  experi- 
ments, using  for  stimulation  a  rheotome  devised  by 
himself  with  a  galvanometer  for  detection  of  response, 
enabled  him  to  plot  the  time  course  of  what  we  now 
call  the  nerve's  action  potential  and  to  determine  its 
latency,  rise-time  and  decay.  One  of  the  pregnant  ob- 
servations he  made  was  that  the  negative  variation 
caused  a  deflection  of  his  galvanometer  that  some- 
times crossed  the  base  line,  thus  exceeding  the  value 
for  the  resting  nerve  potential.  In  today's  terminology, 
he  found  the  overshoot  of  the  action  potential  beyond 
the  resting  potential  level. 

Bernstein  (106)  became  widely  known  for  his 
theory  that  the  membrane  of  the  inactive  fiber  of 
nerve  or  muscle  was  normally  polarized,  having  po.si- 
tive  ions  on  the  outside  and  negative  ions  on  the  in- 
side, and  that  the  action  potential  was  a  self-propa- 
gating depolarization   of  this   membrane.    This   was 

105.  VON  Helmholtz,  H.  (1821-1894).  Messungen  iiber  den 
zcitlichen  Verlauf  der  Zuchung  animalischer  Muskein 
und  die  Fortpflanzungsgeschwindigkeit  der  Reizung  in 
den  Nerven.  Arch.  Anat.  Physiol.  111,  1850. 

106.  Bernstein,  J.  Uber  den  zeitlichen  Verlauf  der  negativen 
Schwankung  des  Nervenstroms.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol,  i :  1 73, 
1868. 


based  on  his  assumption  that  the  membrane  is  se- 
lectivelv  permeable  to  potassium  ions.  His  explana- 
tion of  injury  currents  was  that  they  were  the  result 
of  a  break  in  the  membrane. 

In  the  later  nineteenth  century,  after  a  long  hiatus, 
phvsiology  in  England  was  again  coming  into  its  own. 
At  the  half-century,  which  saw  such  brilliance  in  the 
German  schools,  there  was  virtually  no  physiological 
work  in  progress  in  England.  There  were  no  physi- 
ological laboratories  and  there  was  no  systematic 
physiological  research.  A  dual  chair  in  anatomy  and 
physiology  had  i:)een  created  in  1836  at  University 
College,  London,  and  had  been  given  to  the  anatomist 
William  Sharpey.  Such  teaching  as  he  gave  in  physi- 
ology was  from  books  and  his  pupils  saw  no  experi- 
ments, yet  from  among  them  came  the  leader  of  one 
of  the  more  famous  English  schools  of  physiology, 
Michael  Foster  (1836-1907),  founder  of  the  Cam- 
bridge School.  Though  not  himself  a  neurophysi- 
ologist,  Foster  could  count  among  his  pupils  some  to 
become  later  among  the  most  brilliant  in  the  field, 
Sherrington  (1857-1952),  Gaskell  (1B47-1914),  Lang- 
ley  (1852-1925)  and,  as  descendents  from  the  last, 
Keith  Lucas  and  in  turn  Adrian. 

This,  the  late  nineteenth  century,  was  an  age  of 
great  progress  in  the  development  of  instrumentation 
and,  with  their  improved  tools,  physiologists  were  able 
to  make  more  accurate  observations  of  stimulus 
strength,  response  characteristics  and  time  relation- 
ships than  had  their  predecessors.  In  1871  Bowditch 
(107)  demonstrated  that  heart  muscle  did  not  respond 
with  graded  contractions  to  graded  stimuli.  He  as- 
sumed that  the  global  response  he  observed  was  due 
to  a  leakage  of  excitation  throughout  the  fiber  popu- 
lation of  cardiac  muscle.  It  was  in  fact  the  experi- 
mental evidence  for  what  was  later  to  be  called  the 
'all-or-nothing  law.'  Bowditch,  an  American,  did 
these  experiments  in  Ludwig's  laboratory  in  Leipzig 
where  he  worked  on  the  problem  with  Kronecker,  the 
teacher  of  Harvey  Cushing.  On  his  return  to  Harvard, 
Bowditch  founded  the  first  laboratory  for  physiological 
research  in  the  United  States. 

Forgotten  by  Bowditch,  or  unread,  were  the  writ- 
ings of  Fontana  in  the  eighteenth  century  in  which, 
in  discussing  heart  muscle,  he  said,  "...  the  irritability 
of  the  fibre  can  be  activated  by  a  small  cause,  and  by 
a  feeble  impression :  but  once  activated,  it  has  a 
power  proportional  to  its  own  forces,  which  can  be 

107.  Bowditch,  H.  P.  (1840-191 1).  Uber  die  Eigenthumlich- 
keiten  der  Reizbarkeit  welche  die  Muskelfasern  des 
Herzens  zeigen.  Bcr.  Konigl.  Sachs.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  23: 
652,  1 87 1. 


24 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


much  greater  than  those  of  the  exciting  cause.  .  .  ."'^ 
Fontana  (loS)  went  on  to  a  recognition  of  the  re- 
fractory period  (a  term  introduced  by  Marey)  in 
heart  muscle  which  he  explained  as  an  exhaustion 
of  irritability  resulting  from  the  contraction. 

That  skeletal  muscle  might  share  this  property 
was  also  foreshadowed  by  Fontana  but  did  not  receive 
experimental  proof  until  the  work  of  Fick  (109),  an- 
other pupil  of  Ludwig's,  although  the  finding  was 
not  further  developed  until  the  ingenious  experi- 
ments of  Keith  Lucas  (no)  at  the  beginning  of  this 
century.  In  the  meantime,  an  all-or-nothing  property 
in  nerve  had  been  detected  by  Gotch  (m),  the 
predecessor  of  Sherrington  in  the  Chair  of  Physiology 
in  Liverpool,  a  finding  that  was  to  reach  definitive 
form  in  the  hands  of  Keith  Lucas'  pupil,  Adrian  (i  12, 
113).  That  the  law  applied  to  sensory  as  well  as  to 
motor  nerves  was  established  by  Adrian  &  Forbes 
(114)  in  1922  (in  a  paper  whose  title  replaced  the 
term  'all-or-none'  by  the  more  grammatical  one 
'all-or-nothing').  This  line  of  work  led  on  to  investi- 
gations of  the  refractory  period  of  peripheral  nerve 
and  the  accurate  plotting  of  the  time  course  of  after 
potentials.  The  invention  of  the  vacuum  tube  ampli- 
fier and  the  cathode  ray  oscillo.scope  opened  the 
modern  era  of  electrophysiology,  and  with  them  the 
foundations  of  today's  techniques  were  laid  by 
Gasser  &  Erlanger  (i  15). 

One  branch  of  peripheral  nerve  physiology  remains 

108.  Fontana,  Felice  Caspar  Ferdinand  (1730-1805).  De 
Legibus  Irritabilitatis.  Lucca :  Riccomini,  1 767. 

109.  Fick,  Adolf  (1829- 1901).  Mechanische  Arbeit  und  Wurme- 
fntivicklung  bei  der  Miiskelthritigkeit.  Leipzig:  Brockhaus, 
1882. 

1 10.  Lucas,  K.  The  "all-or-nonc"  contraction  of  ampiiibian 
skeletal  muscle.  J.  Physiol.  38:  113,  igog. 

111.  Gotch,  Francis  (i853-igi3).  The  sub-maximal  electrical 
response   of  nerve    to    a  single   stimulus.  J.    Phyuol.    28: 

395.   1902- 

112.  Adrian,  Edgar  D.  (i88g-  ).  On  the  conduction  of 
subnormal  disturbances  in  normal  nerve.  J.  Physiol.  43: 
389,  1912. 

113.  Adrian,  E.  D.  The  "all-or-none"  principle  in  nerve.  J. 
Physiol.  47:  460,   19 1 4. 

114.  .Adrian,  E.  D.  and  A.  Forbes.  All-or-nothing  responses 
in  sensory  nerve  fibres.  J.  Physiol.  56:  301,  1922. 

115.  Gasser,  Herbert  S.  (1888-  )  and  Joseph  Erlanger 
(1874-  ).  A  study  of  the  action  currents  of  nerve 
with  a  cathode  ray  oscillograph.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  62:  496, 
1922. 

"  Quoted  from  Hoff,  H.  E.  The  history  of  the  refractory 
period.  Yale  J.  Biol.  &  Med.  14:  635,  1942. 


to  be  outlined.  This  is  the  subject  of  neuromu.>;cular 
transmission.  Its  history  is  short  for,  before  the  latter 
half  of  the  nineteenth  century,  continuity  between 
nerve  and  muscle  was  assumed,  the  neuron  theory 
had  not  been  formulated  and  neuroneural  synapsis 
had  not  been  conceived.  The  i  700-year-old  hypothe- 
sis of  a  nervous  fluid  implied  humoral  transmission  in 
structures  having  continuity  and  only  at  mid-nine- 
teenth century,  when  this  was  finally  abandoned,  did 
the  possibility  of  junctional  tissues  become  a  live  one. 

In  1862  Willy  Kiihne  (116,  117),  pupil  of  von 
Briicke  and  later  professor  of  physiology  in  Heidelberg, 
published  a  memoir  on  the  end  organs  of  motor  nerves. 
Noting  the  histological  differences  between  muscle 
and  its  innervating  ner\e,  he  suggested  that  action 
currents  of  the  nerve  by  invasion  of  the  muscle 
caused  it  to  contract.  That  there  was  a  delay  at  the 
neuromuscular  junction  was  noted  in  du  Bois-Rey- 
mond's  laboratory  and  the  master  him.self  considered 
the  possibility  of  a  chemical  influence  (the  agents  he 
mentioned  were  ammonia  and  lactic  acid  which 
Leibig  had  demonstrated  in  muscle  in  1847);  he  went 
to  great  pains,  however,  to  sketch  electrical  fields  in 
support  of  what  was  called  the  'modified  discharge 
hypothesis'  (as  shown  in  fig.  i  7). 

The  controversy  surrounding  the  mode  of  trans- 
mission at  the  motor  end  plate  was  carried  into  the 
modern  era  and,  at  a  time  not  yet  history,  essential 
agreement  was  reached  that  transmission  at  the  neuro- 
muscular junction  is  chemical  in  nature.  The  major 
contribution  that  settled  the  issue  came  from  pharma- 
cological experimentation  of  today's  scientists,  stem- 
ming from  the  pioneer  work  of  Elliott  (118),  Dale 
(119)  and  Loewi  (120)  in  the  early  part  of  the  cen- 
tury. Elliott,  while  a  student  at  Cambridge,  noticed 
that  smooth  muscle  responded  to  adrenin  even  when 
deprived  of  its  sympathetic  nerves  and  this  led  him 

116.  KiJHNE,  Willy  (1837- 1900).  Uber  die  periphniuheii 
Endorgane  der  molorisehrn  .Herven.  Leipzig:  Engelmann, 
1862. 

117.  Kuhne,  VV.  On  the  oiigin  and  causation  of  vital  move- 
ment. Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  44:  427,  1888. 

118.  Elliott,  Thomas  Renton  (1877-  ).  On  the  action 
of  adrenaline.  J.  Physiol.  32:  401,  1905. 

119.  Dale,  Henry  Hallett  (1875-  ).  Transmission  of 
nervous  effects  of  acetylcholine.  Harvey  Lectures  32:  229, 

'937- 

120.  LoEVvi,  Otto  (1873-  ).  Uber  humorale  Ubertrag- 
barkeit  der  Hcrtznervenwirkung.  Arch.  ges.  Physinl.  iBg- 
23g,  1 92 1. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


to  suggest  that  adrcnin  "might  then  be  the  chemical 
stimulant  liberated  on  each  occasion  when  the  im- 
pulse arrives  at  the  periphery."  Langley  (121),  who 
was  at  that  time  professor  of  ph\  siology  at  Cambridge, 
recognizing  that  in  some  smooth  muscle  the  action 
both  of  sympathetic  nerve  stimulation  and  of  adrenin 
was  to  produce  contraction  whereas  in  others  the 
result  was  a  relaxation,  postulated  the  existence  of 
two  kinds  of  receptor  substance — excitatory  and  in- 
hibitory. That  adrenin  mimicked  sympathetic  action 
was  then  accepted. 

The  possibility  of  a  chemical  mediator  for  the 
vagal  action  on  the  heart  was  explored  experi- 
mentally in  several  centers.  Bottazzi  (122),  Martin 
(123)  and  Howell  (124)  thought  the  agent  must  be 
potassium,  Dixon  (125)  that  it  was  muscarine,  an 
alkaloid  closely  related  in  structure  to  the  cholines. 
These  substances  had  been  shown  to  be  active  in  sev- 
eral puzzling  ways.  In  1906  Hunt  &  Taveau  (126) 
had  demonstrated  the  extremely  potent  effect  of 
acetylcholine  on  arterial  pressure,  and  by  1914  the 
work  of  Dale  (127)  was  already  pointing  so  strongly  to 
acetylcholine  being  the  drug  involved  in  parasympa- 
thetic action,  that  he  described  it  as  'parasympatho- 
mimetic' Direct  experimental  proof  was  lacking  that 
a  chemical  substance  excreted  as  a  result  of  nerve 
stimulation  would  in  fact  activate  a  tissue  in  a  similar 
way,  although  the  hypotheses  both  for  epinephrine  in 
the  sympathetic  and  acetvlcholine  in  the  parasympa- 
thetic system  seemed  highly  plausible. 

The  direct  proof  came  from  the  brilliant  researches 
of  Otto  Loewi  (128)  in  which  he  demonstrated  that 

12!.   Langley,  John  Newport  (1852-1906).  On  the  reaction 
of  cells  and  of  nerve-endings  to  certain  poisons,  chiefly  as 
regards  the  reaction  of  striated  muscles  to  nicotine  and  to 
curare.  J.  Physiol.  33:  374,  1905. 
Bottazzi,  P.  Arch.  Physiol.  882,   1896. 
Martin,    E.    G.    The   inhibitory   influence   of  potassium 
chloride   on    tlie   heart,    and    the   eff^ect   of  variations   of 
temperature   upon    this   inhibition   and   upon    vagus   in- 
hibition, .-im.  J.  Physiol.   II  :  370,   1904. 
Howell,  VV.  H.  Vagus  inhibition  of  the  heart  in  its  re- 
lation to  the  inorganic  salts  of  the  blood.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 
15:  280,  1906. 

Dixon,  W.  E.  On  the  mode  of  action  of  drugs.  Med.  Mag. 
16:454,  '907- 

ijfi.  Hunt,  R.  and  R.  de  M.  Taveau.  On  the  physiological 
action  of  certain  cholin  derivatives  and  new  methods  for 
detecting  cholin.  Bril.  A/.  J.  ■2:  1788,  1906. 

127.  Dale,  H.  H.  The  action  of  certain  esters  and  ethers  of 
choline,  and  their  relation  to  muscarine.  J.  Pharmacol. 
&  Exper.    Therap.  6:   147,    1914. 

128.  Loewi,  O.  Uber  humorale  Ubertragbarkeit  Herz- 
nervenwirkung.  .^rch.  ges.  Physiol.   189:  239,   192 1. 


122 
123 


124. 


125- 


^^ 


FIG.  18.  Lift:  an  early  representation  of  spinal  roots  and 
tracts  as  drawn  by  Domenico  Mistichelli  in  his  Traltalo  dtU'.i/io- 
plessia,  1 709  (from  the  copy  in  the  Boston  Medical  Library  by 
courtesy  of  Dr.  Henry  Viets).  Mistichelli  is  considered  to  be 
one  of  the  first  workers  to  recognize  the  crossing  of  the  pyra- 
mids. Right:  the  crossing  of  the  pyramids  was  described  and 
experimentally  demonstrated  on  injury  to  the  brain  in  dogs  by 
Pourfour  du  Petit,  a  pupil  of  Duverney.  His  drawings  are  from 
his  Lettres  d'un  medicin,  1  727.  (From  the  copy  in  the  Bibliotheque 
Nationale.  Reproduction  by  courtesy  of  Dr.  Auguste  Tournay.) 


the  fluid  bathing  a  frog's  heart  which  had  been  stimu- 
lated through  its  vagus  had  an  inhibitory  action  on 
the  beat  of  another  heart.  He  named  the  agent 
'Vagusstoffe.'  From  this  cla.ssic  observation,  one  of 
the  landmarks  of  physiology,  experimentation  spread 
cut  to  the  examination  of  other  tissues,  other  nerves, 
and  other  mediators  and  inhibitors,  and  forms  one  of 
the  wide  fields  of  today's  research.  With  the  recogni- 
tion of  neuroneural  synapses  the  problem  of  trans- 
mission was  carried  from  the  peripheral  neuromuscu- 
lar svstem  into  the  central  nervous  svstem. 


SPINAL    CORD    AND    REFLEX    ACTIVITY 

The  functions  of  the  spinal  cord  long  remained  an 
enigma  to  the  early  physiologists.  For  as  long  as  the 
belief  persisted  that  every  nerve  in  the  body  required 
its  own  canal  leading  directly  from  the  brain  in  order 
to  insure  its  supply  of  animal  spirits,  the  spinal  cord 
appeared  to  be  merely  a  bundle  of  nerve  fibers 
grouped  together.  In  other  words,  it  was  a  prolonga- 
tion of  the  peripheral  nervous  system  channeling  into 
the  brain. 


26 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


The  relationship  of  the  spinal  cord  to  peripheral 
nerves  and  to  the  rest  of  the  central  nervous  system 
could  hardlv  be  understood  until  the  structure  of  the 
neuron  had  been  learned.  The  period  that  saw  the 
great  development  of  knowledge  of  cell  structure  came 
with  the  high-power  microscopes  of  the  nineteenth 
century.  Before  then  descriptions  of  the  finer  elements 
necessarily  lacked  exactness,  though  in  1767  Fontana 
had  given  a  good  account'^  of  the  axis  cylinder,  and 
there  seems  little  reason  to  doubt  that  the  bodies 
Alexander  Monro  (129)  saw  in  the  spinal  cord  in 
1783  were  the  anterior  horn  cells.  Nerve  cells  were 
certainly  seen  by  Dutrochet  (130)  in  1824  though  we 
do  not  find  a  very  exact  description  of  them  before 
1833,  when  Ehrenberg  (131,  132)  published  his  find- 
ings on  the  spinal  ganglia  of  the  frog. 

The  visualization  of  axis  cylinders  on  the  one  hand, 
and  of  cell  bodies  on  the  other,  still  did  not  help  the 
physiologist  very  much  in  his  search  for  understanding 
of  nervous  connections.  It  was  from  the  botanists  that 
the  next  lead  came.  The  cell  theory  had  a  long  history 
among  plant  physiologists  and  its  emphasis  on  the 
role  of  the  nucleus  and  the  cellular  matrix  appealed 
to  microscopists  who  could  see  similar  structures  in 
animal  tissues.  In  1837  Purkinje  (133),  working  at 
home  for  lack  of  a  laboratory  at  the  Universit>-  of 
Breslau  where  he  was  profes.sor,  realized  the  .signifi- 
cance of  the  ob.servations  on  plant  tissues  and  sug- 
gested  that  the  cell  theory  might  justifiably  be  ex- 

129.  Monro,  .\lex.\nder  (secundus)  (1733-1817).  Observa- 
tions on  the  structure  and  Junctions  of  the  nervous  system. 
Edinburgh:  Creech,  1783. 

130.  Dutrochet,  Rene  Joachim  Henri  (i  776-1847).  ^f- 
ckerches  anatomiques  et  physiolooiques  sur  la  structure  intime 
des  animaux  et  des  vegetaux.  Paris :  Bailliere,  1 824. 

131.  Ehrenberg,  C.  G.  Notwendigkeit  einer  feineren  mecha- 
nischen  Zerlegung  des  Gehirns  und  der  Nerven.  Ann. 
Physik.  u.  Chem.  104:  449,  1833. 

132.  Ehrenberg,  C.  G.  Beobachtung  einer  unhekannten  Structur 
des  Seelesorgans.  BerHn,  1836. 

133.  Purkinje,  Johann  Evangelista  (1787-1869).  Uber  die 
gangliose  Natur  bestimmter  Hirntheile.  Ber.  Versamml. 
deutsch.  Natmjorsch.  Artze,  Prague  1837,  p.    175. 

'^  "Le  nerf  est  forme  dun  grand  nombre  de  cylindres 
transparents,  homogenes,  uniformes,  tres-simples.  Ces  cylindres 
paroissent  formes,  comme  d'une  parol,  ou  tunique  tr&  subtile, 
uniforme,  remplie,  autant  I'oeil  peut  enjuger,  dune  humeur 
transparente,  gelatineuse,  insoluble  dans  I'eau.  Chacun  de  ces 
cylindres  recoil  une  enveloppe  en  forme  de  gaine  exterieure, 
la  quelle  est  composee  d'un  nombre  immense  de  fils  torteux." 
Fontana.    Traite  sur  le  venin  de   la  I'ipere.  Florence,  1781.2  vol. 


tended  from  botany  to  zoology.  Two  years  later 
Schwann  (134)  marshalled  the  facts  and  crystallized 
the  idea  in  his  classic  monograph. 

For  an  understanding  of  function,  knowledge  of  the 
cell  bodies  was  not  enough.  The  nerve  tracts  were  of 
primary  importance,  and  during  this  same  period 
histologists  were  finding  that  the  medullated  axon  was 
not  the  only  kind  of  fiber.  In  1838,  in  a  little  book  that 
was  one  of  the  last  scientific  texts  to  be  published  in 
Latin,  Remak  (135)  revealed  the  existence  of  non 
medullated  nerves.  His  work  is  illustrated  by  many 
delicate  drawings  of  cells  from  various  parts  of  the 
nervous  system,  mostly  taken  from  ox  and  man.  But 
by  1865  phy.siologists  knew  that  in  addition  to  medul- 
lated and  nonmedullated  nerves  there  were  other 
fibrous  processes  which  Dieter's  (136)  work  (published 
posthumously)  showed  to  be  dendrites.  In  the  saine 
monograph  there  is  a  description  of  the  glia.  The  cell 
theory  did  not  explain  how  all  these  fibrous  structures 
related  to  the  cell  body,  and  a  student's  thesis  was  one 
of  the  early  publications  to  take  this  step.  In  1842 
von  Helmholtz  (137),  in  the  earliest  of  the  many 
brilliant  contributions  he  made  to  physiology,  estab- 
lished the  connection  between  peripheral  nerve  and 
ganglia  in  invertebrates  using  the  crab,  von  Helmholtz 
was  2 1  years  old  when  he  wrote  this  inaugural  thesis. 

The  next  major  advance  came  in  1850  from  Waller 
(138)  with  his  demonstration  that  axons  degenerate 
when  cut  off  from  their  cell  bodies  and  his  conclusion 
that  the  latter  were  their  source  of  nutriment.  The 
development  by  Marchi  &  Algeri  (139)  of  the  osmic 
acid  stain  for  degenerating  myelin  sheaths  gave  the 
physiologist  a  technique  for  tracing  the  nerve  tracts. 

134.  Schwann,  Theodore  (1810-1882).  Mikroskopische  Unter- 
suchungen  iiber  die  Ubereinstimmung  in  der  Struktur  und  dem 
Wachsthum  der  Thiere  und  Pflanzen.  Berlin:  Sander,  1839; 
English  translation  by  Sydenham  Society,  1847. 

135.  Rem.\k,  Robert  (181 5-1 865).  Observationes  anatomicae  el 
microscopuae  de  systematis  nervosi  structura.  Berlin:  Reimer, 
1838. 

136.  Dieters,  Otto  Friedrich  Karl  (1821-1863).  Unter- 
suchungen  iiber  Gehirn  und  Riickenmark  des  Menschen  und  der 
Sdugetiere.  Brunswick:  Vieweg,  1863. 

137.  VON  Helmholtz,  H.  De  Fabrica  systematis  nervosi  Everte- 
bratorum  (Inaugural  Thesis).    1842. 

138.  Waller,  .Augustus  Volney  (1816-1870).  Experiments 
on  the  section  of  the  glossopharyngeal  and  hypoglossal 
nerves  of  the  frog,  and  observations  of  the  alterations 
produced  thereby  in  the  structure  of  their  primitive 
fibres.  Phil.  Trans.   140:  432,  1850. 

139.  Marchi,  V.  and  C.  Algeri.  Sulle  degenerazioni  discen- 
denti  consecutive  a  lesioni  della  corteccia  cerebrale.  Riv. 
sper.   Frernat   H:  492,    1885. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


27 


TAB  11;    1 


FIG.  iq.  Left:  Jiri  Pfochaska  of  Prague,  the  proponent  of 
automatic  reflexion  in  the  medulla  and  spinal  cord.  Ri«ht: 
Prochaska's  illustration  of  the  spinal  roots  and  their  ganglia. 

The  definitive  study  of  the  relationship  of  the  medul- 
lated  axon  to  the  nerve  cell  followed  in  1889  and  was 
the  work  of  von  Kolliker  (140),  profes.sor  of  anatomy 
in  Wiirzburg.  From  this  wealth  of  accumulated 
knowledge,  a  generalized  concept  of  neuron  behavior 
became  possible  and  in  i8gi  a  clear  formulation  was 
achieved  by  Waldeyer-Hartz  (141).  The  neuron 
theory  was  established.  In  reviewing  thfse  basic  steps 
that  had  to  be  taken  before  any  unravelling  of  central 
nervous  system  pathways  could  proceed  with  cer- 
tainty, one  is  struck  by  the  fact  that  so  many  of  the 
contributors  (Schwann,  Remak,  von  Helinholtz, 
Kolliker)  were  pupils  of  Johannes  Miiller. 

Another  of  the  early  stumbling  blocks  to  an  under- 
standing of  the  spinal  cord  was  the  difTerentiation  of 
motor  and  sensory  function.  It  was  early  suspected 
that  the  ganglia  of  the  spinal  roots  were  in  some  way 
involved  in  this  question.  Galen  had  thought  that  the 
presence  of  a  ganglion  indicated  that  the  nerve  was 
powerfully  motor  in  action  and  here  the  matter  rested 
for  some  centuries.  In  1783  Alexander  Monro  (129) 
noted   that  the  spinal   ganglia  were  formed  on   the 

140.  VON    Kolliker,    Rudolf    Albert    (1817-1905).    Mikro- 

skopische  Analomie.   Leipzig,    1850- 1854. 
14L  Waldever-Hartz,     Heinrich      Wilhelm     Gottfried 

(1836- 1 921).  Uber  einige  neuere  Forschungen  im  Gebiete 

der    Anatomic   des    Centralnervensystems.    Deutsche   med. 

Wchnschr.  17:  12 13,  1244,   1287,   1331,   1352,   189L 


posterior  roots  and  that  their  coalescence  with  the 
anterior  roots  occurred  peripherally  to  these  swellings. 
But  like  Galen  he  thought  that  they  were  concerned 
with  'muscular'  nerves  and  defended  them  as  such 
against  the  suggestion  by  James  Johnstone  (142)  that 
their  action  was  to  cut  ofT  sensation.  This  rather 
bizarre  concept  had  received  .some  consideration  in 
the  mid-eighteenth  century. 

The  presence  of  ganglia  suggested  to  several  minds 
a  specialization  of  function  in  the  nerves  on  which 
they  were  formed.  Both  Prochaska  C'43)  3"^ 
Soemmering  (144)  had  drawn  attention  to  the  re- 

142.  Johnstone,  James  (i 730-1802).  Essay  on  the  use  of  the 
ganglions  of  the  nerves.  Phil.  Trans.  54:  177,  1765. 

143.  Prochaska,  Jiri  (1749-1820).  De  Structura  .Nervorum. 
Prague:  Gerle,  1780-1784.  3  vol. 

144.  Soemmering,  Samuel  Thomas  (1755-1830).  De  bast 
encephali  et  originibus  nervorum  cranio  egredienlum.  Got- 
tingen :  Vandenhoeck,  1778. 


28 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


semblance  between  the  ganglia  of  the  fifth  cranial 
nerves  and  those  of  the  posterior  roots,  and  Bichat 
(145),  the  brilliant  French  pathologist  who  died  so 
young,  had  gone  so  far  as  to  associate  all  ganglia  with 
the  nervous  processes  of  involuntary,  unconscious 
'organic'  life. 

The  differentiation  between  the  ganglia  found  in 
the  sympathetic  nersous  system  and  those  on  the 
roots  of  the  central  nervous  system  was  to  come  later. 
Charles  Bell  made  the  distinction  but  admitted  he  did 
not  know  what  role  was  pla\ed  b\-  the  .sympathetic 
nerves  or  by  their  ganglia  (146).  His  many  studies  on 
the  fifth  and  seventh  cranial  nerves  (146-148),  illus- 
trated by  his  own  beautiful  drawings,  are  classics,  and 
his  demonstrations  of  the  function  in  the  nerves  of 
the  face  are  perpetuated  in  the  name  Bell's  palsy. 
Bell  had  come  from  Edinburgh  to  the  famous  ana- 
tomical school  that  William  Hunter  had  founded  in 
Great  Windmill  Street  near  Piccadilh'.  A  brilliant 
di.ssector  but  not  primarily  an  experimentalist.  Bell 
relied  heavily  on  his  brother-in-law,  John  Shaw,  in 
this  aspect  of  his  work  and  suffered  a  great  loss  when 
Shaw  died. 

In  the  cord  the  various  columns  had  been  dissected 
by  the  anatomists  and  the  grouping  together  of  nerves 
in  such  larue  bundles  had  certainly  seemed  suggestive 
of  parcellation  of  function.  But  not  all  anatomists 
were  agreed.  Bichat  on  dissecting  out  some  nerve 
filaments  found  them  centrally  located  in  the  lower 
cord  but  more  lateral  higher  up.  He  therefore  con- 
cluded that  although  the  filaments  had  individual 
properties,  the  fasciculi  were  mixed.  The  idea  per- 
sisted, however,  that  the  columns  and  also  the  spinal 
roots  might  have  different  functions  according  to 
whether  they  were  anterior  or  posterior.  An  early  idea 
was  that  the  anterior  roots  carried  Ijoth  motor  and 
sensory  supplies  for  the  muscles  while  the  posterior 
roots  gave  a  sensory  service  for  the  skin.  An  Edinburgh 

145.  Bichat,  Marie  Francois  Xavier  (1771-1802).  Aualomie 
generale,  appliquee  a  la  physiologie  el  a  la  medecine.  Paris: 
Brosson,  1801,  2  vol.,  English  translation  by  G.  Hay  ward 
Boston:  Richardson  and  Lord,    1822.  3  vol. 

146.  Bell,  Charles  (1774-1842).  The  Nervous  System  of  the 
Human  Body  as  explained  in  a  series  of  papers  read  be/ore 
the  Royal  Society  0]  London.  Edinburgh:  Black,  1836. 

147.  Bell,  C.  On  the  nerves;  giving  an  account  of  some  ex- 
periments on  their  structure  and  functions,  which  lead 
to  a  new  arrangement  of  the  system.  Phil.  Trans.  1 1 1  :  398, 
1 82 1 . 

148.  Bell,  C.  Of  the  nerves  which  as.sociatc  the  muscles  of  the 
chest  in  the  actions  of  breathing,  .speaking,  and  expres- 
sion. Being  a  continuation  of  the  paper  on  the  structure 
and  functions  of  the  nerves.  Phil.  Trans.  112:  284,  1822. 


anatomist,  Alexander  Walker  Ci49)>  suspected  that 
they  might  serve  .separate  roles  but  unfortunately 
picked  the  posterior  root  as  the  motor  and  the 
anterior  root  as  sensory. 

In  181  I  a  small  pamphlet  was  pri\-ately  primed, 
entitled  Idea  oj  o  new  anatomy  of  the  brain  suhmitled  jir 
the  observation  of  his  friends.  The  author  was  Charles 
Bell  (150).  This  pamphlet  had  no  general  distribution, 
no  more  than  100  copies  being  printed.  (Only  three 
are  known  to  exist  today,  one  of  which  is  in  the  Na- 
tional Library  of  Medicine  in  Washington;  in  Eng- 
land, copies  can  be  seen  at  the  British  Museum  and 
at  the  Royal  Society.)  Bell  stated  that  the  purpose  of 
this  pamphlet  was  to  assure  his  friends  that  in  his  dis- 
sections of  the  brain  he  was  investigating  its  structure 
and  not  searching  for  the  seat  of  the  soul.  In  this  work 
he  stated  his  opinion  that  nerves  owe  their  differences 
in  properties  to  their  being  connected  to  different 
parts  of  the  brain.  He  said  that,  holding  this  opinion, 
he  wondered  whether  the  double  roots  of  the  spinal 
nerves  might  indicate  that  "nerves  of  different  en- 
dowments were  in  the  same  cord,  and  held  together 
by  the  same  sheath."  To  test  this  idea  experimentally, 
he  cut  "across  the  posterior  fasciculus"  and  noted 
that  there  were  no  convulsive  movements  of  the 
muscles  of  the  back;  but  that  on  touching  the  anterior 
fa.sciculus  with  the  point  of  a  knife,  the  muscles  of  the 
back  were  immediately  convulsed.  From  this  experi- 
ment he  concluded  at  that  time,  "The  spinal  nerves 
being  double,  and  having  their  roots  in  the  spinal 
marrow,  of  which  a  portion  comes  from  the  cerebrum 
and  a  portion  from  the  cerebellum,  they  convey  the 
attributes  of  both  grand  divisions  of  the  brain  to  every 
part,  and  therefore  the  distribution  of  such  nerves  is 
simple,  one  nerve  supplying  its  distinct  part." 

It  may  be  noted  that  there  is  in  this  pamphlet  no 
suggestion  that  the  posterior  columns  or  roots  might 
be  sensory  in  function.  Bell  considered  the  cerebellum 
to  be  concerned  with  involuntary  and  unconscious 
functions  ("the  .secret  operation  of  the  bodily  frame" 
and  "the  operation  of  the  viscera")  whereas  he  recog- 
nized the  cerebrum  "as  the  grand  organ  by  which 
the  mind  is  united  with  the  body.  Into  it  all  the  nerves 
from  the  external  organs  of  the  senses  enter;  and  from 
it  all  the  nerves  which  are  agents  of  the  will  pass  out." 

149.  Walker,  Alexander  (1779-1852).  New  anatomy  and 
physiology  of  the  brain  in  particular,  and  of  the  nervous 
system  in  general,  -irch.  Universal  Sc.  3:  172,  1809 

1  fjo.  Bell,  C.  Idea  of  a  new  anatomy  of  the  brain  submitted  for  the 
observation  of  his  friends.  Privately  printed,  1811;  repro- 
duced in  J.  F.  Fulton.  Selected  Readings  in  the  History  of 
Physiology.  Springfield:  Thomas,  1930,  p.  251. 


THE  HISTORICAL  DEVELOPMENT  OF  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


29 


In  essence,  thcrcroic,  Bell  regarded  the  cerebellum, 
posterior  columns  and  posterior  spinal  roots  as  con- 
cerned with  unconscious  impressions  and  involuntary 
movements;  the  cerebrum,  anterior  columns  and 
anterior  roots  as  conveying  conscious  sensation  and 
willed  moxements. 

On  Julv  22,  1822,  Frangois  Magendie,  member  of 
the  Academy  of  Sciences  of  Paris  (and  later  to  be 
professor  at  the  College  de  France),  read  a  paper  (151) 
to  the  Academy  as  a  result  of  which  the  following 
entry  was  made:  "M.  Magendie  reports  the  discovery 
he  has  recently  made,  that  if  the  posterior  roots  of  the 
spinal  nerves  are  cut,  only  the  sensation  of  those  nerves 
is  abolished,  and  if  the  anterior  roots  arc  cut,  only  the 
movements  they  cause  are  lost."  This  report  was 
followed  by  a  fuller  account  (152,  153)  in  the  journal 
that  Magendie  himself  had  founded.  The  experiments, 
made  on  puppies  which  survi\ed  the  surgical  pro- 
cedures, gave  Magendie  the  confidence  to  state  "that 
the  anterior  and  posterior  roots  of  the  nerves  which 
arise  from  the  spinal  marrow,  have  different  func- 
tions, that  the  posterior  appear  more  particularly 
destined  to  sensibility,  whilst  the  anterior  seem  more 
especially  allied  to  motion." 

In  spite  of  his  not  having  suggested  a  function  of 
conscious  sensation  for  the  posterior  roots  in  either 
the  privately  printed  pamphlet  or  published  papers 
(147)  on  the  fifth  and  seventh  cranial  nerves,  Bell 
with  a  questionable  lack  of  scruple  claimed  full  pri- 
ority and  engaged  in  a  wrangle  that  invaded  the  scien- 
tific journals  for  many  years.  This  carried  the  un- 
pleasant flavor  of  evidence  twisted  by  hindsight.  Bell's 
"republications'  in  1824  (154)  of  his  earlier  writings 
contained  .subtle  changes  in  wording  that  deceived 
his  supporters  into  believing  his  claims  to  be  better 
founded  than  they  were.^^  Among  those  hoodwinked 
were  Flourens  and,  at  first,  Magendie's  pupil,  Claude 
Bernard.    Posterity  gives  each  some  credit    by   pre- 

151.  Magendie,  Franqols  (1783-1855).  Proces-verb,  1822. 
Acad.  Sc.  7:  348,  1820-1823. 

152.  Magendie,  F.  Experiences  sur  les  fonctions  des  racines 
des  nerfs  rachidicns.  J.  phystol.  exper.  et  path.  2;  276,  1822. 

153.  Magendie,  F.  Experiences  sur  les  fonctions  des  racines 
des  nerfs  qui  naissent  de  la  moelle  epiniere.  J.  physiol. 
exper.  et  path.  2:  366,  1822.  [References  152  and  153  can 
be  read  in  English  in  Alexander  Walker's  translations 
in.  Documents  and  dales  of  modern  discoveries  in  the  nerv- 
ous   system    (Pub.     anonymously)      London:     Churchill, 

1 839-] 

154.  Bell,  C.  An  Exposition  of  the  Natural  System  of  the  Nerves  of 
the  Human  Body  with  a  Republication  of  the  Papers  Delivered 
to  the  Royal  Society,  on  the  Subject  of  .Verves.  London:  .Spot- 
tiswoode,  1824. 


FIG.  20.  The  protagonists  in  the  Bell-Magendie  controversy. 
Bell  (/f//)  and  Magendie  Qright}  as  young  men.  The  portrait 
of  Bell  was  painted  by  .Antony  Stewart  of  Edinburgh  in  1804; 
that  of  Magendie  (attributed  to  Guerin)  is  at  the  College  de 
France. 


serving  the  nomenclature  of  the  Bell-Magendie  Law. 
In  spite  of  his  claims.  Bell  made  no  move  to  get  ex- 
perimental proof  of  the  function  of  the  posterior  roots 
and  as  late  as  1832  (155)  was  stressing  that  their 
sensory  nature  was  only  inferred.  He  said  in  his  lec- 
tures to  the  Royal  College  of  Physicians,  ".  .  .as  we 
have  proved  the  anterior  column  to  be  the  origin  of 
the  motor  nerves,  we  may  infer  the  posterior  roots  are 
those  which  render  the  entire  nerve  a  nerve  of  sensa- 
tion." In  1844  Johannes  Miiller  (156)  confirmed  the 
law  experimentally,  something  Bell  had  never  done, 
but  the  conclusion  seems  inescapable  that  the  concept 
in  its  complete  form  as  well  as  its  experimental  proof 
was  first  contributed  by  Magendie. 

Magendie,  whose  youth  coincided  with  the  French 
Re\olution,  came  from  surgery  into  physiology  where 
his  urge  towards  experimentation  could  give  him 
greater  satisfaction.  So  strongly  empiricist  was  he  that 
he  rarely  made  generalizations  from  his  observations 

155.  Bell,  C.  Lectures  on  the  physiology  of  the  brain  and 
nervous  system.  Reported  in:  Ryan's  Med.  .Surg.  J.  i: 
682,  752,  1832. 

156.  MtJLLER,  J.  Bestutigung  des  Bell'schen  Lehrsatzes.  Notiz. 
a.  d.  Geb.  d.  natur-  u.  heilk.  (Weimar)  30:  113,  1831; 
this  is  more  readily  available  in  French  in  Ann.  Sc.  Natur. 
23:  95,  1831,  and  a  section  is  translated  into  English  in 
\V.  Stirling.  Some  Apostles  of  Physiology.  London :  Waterlow, 
1902. 


'^  For  a  detailed  comparison  of  the  texts  see  Flint,  A.  Con- 
siderations historiques  sur  les  proprietes  des  racines  des  nerfs 
rachidiens.  J.  de  ianat.  et  de  physiol.  5:  520,  1868. 


30 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


in  the  laboratory  which  were  many  and  varied.  His 
work  on  the  spinal  roots  led  him  to  follow  the  differ- 
entiation of  function  into  the  spinal  tracts  where  he 
found  that  pressure  on  the  posterior  columns,  but  not 
on  the  anterior,  caused  signs  of  pain.  One  other  aspect 
of  Magendie's  work  on  the  spinal  cord  should  be 
mentioned,  his  rediscovery  of  the  cerebrospinal  fluid 
(157).  Sixty  years  earlier  this  had  been  seen  and 
described  by  Cotugno  (158),  at  that  time  a  young 
physician  in  the  Hospital  for  Incurables  in  Naples, 
but  his  monograph  had  stirred  no  general  interest 
though  it  helped  to  win  him  the  chair  of  anatomy  at 
the  university.  Magendie  described  the  forainen 
known  by  his  name,  but  oddly  revived  a  valve-like 
role  for  the  pineal  as  controller  of  this  opening.  He 
thought  the  fluid  was  secreted  by  the  arachnoid  mem- 
brane, and  it  was  many  years  later  that  its  origin  in 
the  choroid  plexuses  was  discovered.  A  later  pamphlet 
by  Magendie  (159)  on  the  cerebrospinal  fluid  has 
some  fine  illustrations  by  H.  Jacob. 

Once  the  differentiation  of  function  between  the 
anterior  and  posterior  roots  had  been  accepted,  the 
finer  points  as  to  which  regions  were  inner\ated  by 
their  fibers  began  to  occupy  the  physiologists.  The 
question  as  to  whether  all  the  fibers  of  an  anterior 
root  served  the  same  or  many  muscles  was  paralleled 
by  its  corollary  as  to  whether  one  muscle  received 
fibers  from  one  or  many  roots.  That  the  last  arrange- 
ment is  the  correct  one  was  first  clearly  shown  by 
Eckhardt  (160)  in  frogs  and  by  Peyer  (161)  in  rabbits. 
Both  were  working  in  Carl  Ludwig's  laboratory.  The 
definitive  demonstrations  came  later  from  Sherring- 
ton's (162)  careful  analyses,  mostly  in  the  monkey, 
from  which  he  concluded  that  '"the  position  of  the 

157.  Magendie.  Memoire  sur  la  liquide  qui  se  trouve 
dans  le  crane  et  canal  vertebral  de  Thomme  et  des 
animaux  mammifiercs.  J.  physiut.  cxph.  el  path.  5:  27, 
1825. 

158.  Cotugno,  Domenico  (i  736-1822).  De  Ischiade  Nervosa 
Commentarius.  Naples:  Simonios,  1764;  a  portion  has  been 
translated  into  English.  A  Treatise  on  the  Nervous  Sciatica^ 
or  Nervous  Hip  Gout.  London:  Wilkie,  1775,  p.  14. 

159.  Magendie,  F.  Rkherches  physiologiques  et  cliniques  sur  le 
liquide  cephalorachidien  ou  cerebro-spinal.  Paris:  Mequignon- 
Marvis,  1842. 

160.  Eckhardt,  C.  Uber  Reflexbcwcgungender  vier  letzten 
Nervenpaare  des  Frosches.  ^Ischr.  rat.  Med.  (ist  series) 
I  :  281,  1849. 

161.  Peyer,  J.  Uber  die  pcriphcrischcn  Endigungen  der 
motorischen  und  sensibelen  Fasern  der  in  den  Plexus 
brachialis  des  Kaninchens  eintretenden  Nerven  wurzeln. 
Zlschr.  rat.  Med.  (ist  series)  4:  67,  1853. 

162.  Sherrington,  Charles  Scott  (1857- 1952).  Notes  on 
the  arrangement  of  some  motor  fibres  in  the  lumbosacral 
plexus.  J.  Physiol.   13:  621,  1892. 


nerve-cells  sending  motor  fibres  to  any  one  skeletal 
muscle  is  a  scattered  one,  extending  throughout  the 
whole  length  of  the  spinal  segments  innervating  that 
muscle." 

Tracing  of  the  fibers  of  the  sensory  roots  was  in- 
trinsically more  difficult.  Tiirck's  (163)  studies  in 
X'ienna  had  indicated  the  complexity  of  sensory  inner- 
vation in  the  dog,  and  Herringham  (164)  had  found 
the  segmental  relationship  with  the  vertebrae;  but 
again  it  was  Sherrington  (165)  who,  using  the  reflex 
as  criterion  of  the  existence  of  afferent  fibers,  un- 
ravelled the  phenomena  of  overlapping  of  segmental 
cutaneous  innervation.  Until  the  time  of  Sherrington 
it  had  been  thought  that  the  motor  fibers  to  a  given 
muscle  were  derived  from  the  same  spinal  segment 
that  received  the  sensory  inflow  from  the  skin  sur- 
rounding it.  This  was  particularly  the  view  of  Krause 
(166).  Sherrington's  mapping  of  myotomes  and  der- 
matomes showed  this  rule  to  be  erroneous. 

.Sherrington's  development  of  a  comprehensive 
theory  of  reflex  action  could  scarcely  have  been  en- 
visaged before  the  sensory  endings  in  muscle  had  been 
discovered.  This  advance  was  mainly  the  work  of 
Rufiini  (167,  168)  who  in  1892  identified  as  sensory 
organs  muscle  spindles,  tendon  organs  and  Pacinian 
(169)  corpuscles.  These  structures  had  been  seen  and 
dcscriljed  by  others,  but  their  function  had  not  been 
appreciated.  The  need  for  an  apparatus  for  muscle 
sense  had  been  felt  by  Charles  Bell  (170)  in  order  to 
convey  "a  sense  of  the  condition  of  the  muscles  to  the 
brain,"  and  he  postulated  "a  circle  of  nerves,"  saying 
that  "every  muscle  has  two  nei^es,  of  different  proper- 
ties supplied  to  it."  That  sensations  are  aroused  by 

163.  TiJRCK,  Ludwig  (1810-1868).  Uber  die  Haut-Sensibili- 
tatsbewirke  der  enzelnen  Riickenmarksnervenpaare. 
Denkschr.  Akad.   Wiss.  29:  299,   1868. 

164.  Herringham,  VV.  P.  The  minute  anatomy  of  the  brachial 
plexus.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  41:  423,  1887. 

165.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Experiments  in  examination  of  the 
peripheral  distribution  of  the  fibres  of  the  posterior  roots 
of  some  spinal  nerves.  Phil.   Trans.  184  B;  641,  1894. 

166.  Krause,  Fedor  (1856-1937).  Beitrdge  zur  Neurologie  der 
oberen  Extremildt.  Leipzig,   1865. 

167.  RuFFiNi,  Angelo.  Di  una  particolare  reticella  nervosa  e 
di  alcuni  corpuscoli  del  Pacini  che  si  trovano  in  conces- 
sione  cogli  organi  musculo  tendinei  del  gatto.  Atti  R. 
Accad.  Lincei  1  :  12,  1889;  French  translation  in:  Sur  un 
reticule  nervcux  special  et  sur  quelques  corpuscles  de 
Pacini  qui  se  trouvent  en  connexion  avec  les  organes 
musculo-tendineux  du  chat.  Arch.  ital.  biol.  18:  loi,  1893. 

168.  RuFFlNi,  .\.  Observations  on  sensory  nerve  endings  in 
voluntary  muscles.  Brain  20:  368,  1897. 

169.  Pacini,  Filipfo  (181 2-1883).  -^uovi  organi  scoperti  net 
corpo  humani.  Pistoja:  Cino,    1840. 

170.  Bell,  C.  On  the  nervous  circle  which  connects  the  volun- 
tary muscles  with  the  brain.  Phil.    Tram.  2:   172,   1826. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


31 


movements  of  the  limbs  is  an  oljservation  that  goes 
back  at  least  to  Descartes'  posthumous  treatise  (171), 
but  that  the  act  of  volition  in  itself  could  also  be  'felt' 
was  an  idea  espoused  by  some,  including,  rather  sur- 
prisingly, von  Helmholtz."  But  a  peripheral  rather 
than  a  central  mechanism  had  more  adherents  for, 
like  Bichat,  they  thought  that  muscles  must  be  sensi- 
tive. 

Infiltrating  the  early  work  on  spinal  cord  physi- 
ology is  the  gradual  development  of  the  idea  of  the 
reflex.  The  eventual  emergence  of  a  concept  of  reflex 
activity  grew  out  of  centuries  of  attempts  to  explain 
animal  movements,  motion  receiving  more  attention 
than  sensation  for  it  was  considered  to  be  the  sign  of 
life.  Galen  had  regarded  movements  as  three  in  kind : 
natural  (such  as  the  pulse),  governed  by  the  heart; 
voluntary,  governed  by  the  soul  (located  in  the  brain); 
and  unconscious  movements  of  voluntary  muscles 
(such  as  in  respiration).  Involuntary  muscle  was  un- 
known even  in  the  days  of  Fernel  (i  72)  and  Descartes 
(i  73),  both  of  whom  emphasized  a  distinction  between 
movements  dictated  by  reason  and  those  due  to  the 
appetites.  The  ideas  of  Fernel  and  of  Descartes  have 
both  long  been  regarded  as  forerunners  of  the  concept 
of  reflex  activity.  The  claims  for  Fernel  rest  on  his 
observation  of  automatic  movements,  some  of  which 
we  now  know  to  be  reflexly  initiated;  but  the  pe- 
ripheral origin  or  the  stimulus  that  caused  them  was 
not  recognized  by  him.  An  ardent  supporter  of 
Descartes  as  the  originator  was  du  Bois-Reymond 
(174)  who  stressed  this  claim  in  his  eulogy  of  Miiller, 
written  at  the  time  of  the  latter's  death. 

The  first  suggestion  that  perhaps  the  spinal  cord 
could  be  a  center  for  communication  between  nerves 
was  made  by  Thomas  Willis  (i  75)  who  came  very 
close  to  picturing  the  reflex.  He  thought  that  all 
voluntary  movements  came  from  the  cerebrum,  all 
involuntary  from  the  cerebellum  and  that  they  were 
ruled  by  a  soul  that  resided  both  in  the  blood  and  in 
the  nervous  fluid.  For  Willis  the  medulla  was  an 
appendix  of  the  brain  which  he  likened  to  a  musical 
organ  (30)  taking  air  into  its  bellows  (i.e.  animal 
spirits  from  the  brain)  in  order  to  blow  them  out  into 

171.  Descartes,  R.  Traite  de  I' Homme,  first  French  ed.  1664, 
chapt.  77. 

172.  Fernel,  J.  De  Naiurali  Parte  Medicinae  (ist  ed.).  Paris: 
Simon  de  Colines,  1542;  2nd  ed.  Physiologia.  1554. 

173.  Descartes,  R.  Traite  de  I' Homme,  first  French  cd.  1664. 

174.  DU  Bois-Reymond,  E.  Gedachnissrede  auf  Johannes  Miiller. 
Berlin,  1858;  reprinted  in  Reden,  vol.  2.  Leipzig:  Veit, 
1887. 

175.  Willis,  Thomas  (i 621 -1675).  De  Anima  Brutorum  {De 
Scientia  seu  Cognitione  brutorum^.  London:  Davis,  1672. 


the  appropriate  organ  pipes  (the  nerves).  Elsewhere 
(176)  Willis  showed  his  interest  in  the  organ  as  a 
musical  instrument  and  gave  some  description  of  it. 

Where  Willis  came  close  to  describing  reflex  action 
was  in  stating  that  sen.se  impres.sions  carried  by  the 
animal  spirits  to  the  sensorium  commune  (which  he 
put  in  the  corpus  striatum)  went  on  to  higher  levels 
of  the  cerebrum  where  they  were  perceived  and 
formed  into  memories.  Some,  however,  were  reflected 
back  towards  the  muscles  ('species  alia  reflexa').  Al- 
though the  resultant  movement  was  automatic  and 
although  one  might  be  unaware  of  the  sensory  stimu- 
lus, Willis  held  that  one  was  conscious  of  the  resultant 
muscular  effect.  The  example  he  gives  is  irritation  of 
the  stomach  causing  vomiting,  and  it  is  noticeable 
that  Willis's  discussion  of  'reflexes'  comes  in  his  chap- 
ter on  knowledge  and  recognition. 

Willis  used  'motus  reflexus'  and  the  verb  refluere' 
in  making  this  proposition  and  the  terms  were  used 
again  by  Baglivi  (i  77)  who  refers  to  him.  Their  usage 
of  'reflexus'  reads  as  though  it  were  closer  to  the 
modern  term  than  Descartes'  'esprits  reflechis'.'* 
Across  the  centuries  the  changing  nuances  of  word 
meanings  make  it  impossible  to  catch  the  exact  conno- 
tation intended  by  an  author,  but  Descartes'  interest 
in  the  reflection  of  light  rays  suggests  that  this  may 
have  been  the  analogy  he  had  in  mind. 

A  mechanism  for  the  mediation  of  involuntary 
movements  was  not  the  only  one  for  which  physiolo- 
gists were  .searching.  The  early  workers  were  much 
exercised  by  what  they  termed  'the  sympathy  of  parts' 
for  they  recognized  an  integration  of  body  mecha- 
nisms that  eluded  nervous  influence  flowing  only  from 
the  brain.  Some  suggested  an  interaction  taking  place 
peripherally  in  a  plexus,  an  anastomosis  of  the 
sensory   and   motor   nerve   endings.    Winslow  (178), 

176.  Ibid.,  chapt.  6. 

177.  B.'\GLivi,  Giorgio  (1668-1707).  De  fibra  moirue.  1700, 
book  I,  chapt.  5. 

178.  Winslow,  James  Benignus  (1669-1760).  Exposition 
anatomique  de  la  structure  du  corps  humain.  Paris:  Duprez 
and  Desessartz,  1732,  pt.  VI  (illustrated  by  plates  from 
Bartolomeo  Eustachius  (i 520-1 574).  Tabulae  anatomicae. 
Rome:  Gonzaga,  171 4);  English  translation  by  G.  Doug- 
las. Edinburgh:  Donaldson  &  Elliot,  1772.   2  vol. 


"  In  discussing  the  sensation  of  outward  movement  of  an 
eyeball  the  external  rectus  of  which  is  paralyzed,  he  says,  "We 
feel,  then  what  impulse  of  the  will,  and  how  strong  a  one,  we 
apply  to  turn  the  eye  to  a  given  position."  von  Helmholtz, 
H.  Handbuch  der  physiologischen  Optik.  Leipzig:  Voss,  1867, 
parts  translated  into  English  by  William  James  in  his  Principles 
of  Psychology. 

"  Descartes  used  this  term  only  once,  in  Passions  de  I'Ame. 


32 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


working  in  Paris  and  later  in  Copenhagen,  thought 
he  had  found  the  clue  in  the  ganglia  of  the  sympa- 
thetic chain.  These  he  envisaged  as  small  brains  in 
wliich  intercommunication  between  nerves  could  take 
place,  efTecting  sympathy  between  various  visceral 
organs.  "These  ganglions  .  .  .  may  be  looked  upon," 
he  said,  "as  so  many  origins  or  gcrmina  dispersed 
through  this  great  pair  of  nerves,  and  consequently  as 
so  many  little  brains."  This  ingenious  but  erroneous 
theory  has  left  its  name  on  the  structures,  the  sympa- 
thetic ganglia.  Winslow  illustrated  his  te.xt  with  the 
fine  plates  of  Eustachius  that  had  lain  for  so  long  un- 
noticed in  the  \'atican  Library.  These  plates  do  not 
however  show  the  'small  brains.' 

In  following  the  early  ideas  about  'sympathy  be- 
tween the  parts'  it  must  be  remembered  that,  although 
so  much  emphasis  was  laid  on  the  humors  by  early 
physiologists,  endocrines  were  unknown  and  conse- 
quently their  influence  could  not  be  in\oked.  There 
were,  however,  all  down  the  centuries,  some  who  held 
that  the  blood  was  the  great  integrator.  In  the 
eighteenth  century,  for  example,  John  Hunter  (179) 
was  teaching  that  the  blood  was  the  agent  of  sym- 
pathy.'^ He  was  drawn  to  this  view  from  his  work  on 
inflammation  and  fevers  arising  from  gunshot  wounds 
in  the  soldiers  he  cared  for  as  an  army  surgeon  in  the 
Seven  Years'  War  with  France. 

Only  slowly  did  the  concept  of  reflex  activity  gain 
ground.  Hunter's  contemporary  and  fellow  Scot, 
Robert  VVhytt,  was  accumulating  observations  and 
making  experiments  that  are  fundamental  to  modern 
physiology,  although  his  descriptions  of  them  are  also 
often  cloaked  by  his  terminology.  In  the  first  place 
(180),  he  recognized  the  in\oluntary  nature  of  pupil- 
lary contraction  and  dilation  and  demonstrated  the 
dependence  of  this  action  on  the  integrity  of  the 
corpora  quadrigemina,  thus  anticipating  the  work  of 
Herbert  Mayo  (181)  in  the  next  century.  He  went  on 

179.  Hunter,  John  (1728-1793).  Trealise  on  the  Blood,  Inflam- 
mation and  Gunshot  Wounds.  London :  Nicol,  1 794. 

180.  Whytt,  Robert  (17 14- 1766).  An  essay  on  the  vital  and 
other  involuntary  motions  of  the  animal.  Edinburgh:  Hamil- 
ton, Balfour  and  Neill,  1751. 

181.  Mavo,  Herbert  (1796- 1852).  Anatomical  and  Phynological 
Commentaries.  London;  Underwood,  vol.  I,  1822;  vol.  II, 
■  823. 

"Samuel  Taylor  Coleridge's  comment  on  some  of  John 
Hunter's  writings  is  perhaps  a  little  harsh:  "The  light  which 
occasionally  flashes  upon  us  seems  at  other  times,  to  struggle 
through  an  unfriendly  medium,  and  even  sometimes  to  suffer 
a  temporary  occultation."  Coleridge,  S.  T.  Hints  towards  the 
Formation  oj  a  more  Comprehensive  Theory  of  Life.  Philadelphia: 
Lea  &  Blanchard,  1848. 


to  the  study  of  in\oluntarv  movements  of  voluntary 
muscle  systems  in  decapitated  animals.  The  move- 
ments of  animals  after  their  heads  had  been  severed 
was  common  knowledge  to  every  housewife  who  had 
ever  killed  a  chicken  and  had  attracted  the  attention 
of  scientists  since  Leonardo's  day.  Even  in  the  seven- 
teenth century  Boyle  C'82)  had  recognized  the  impli- 
cations of  these  phenomena,  realizing  that  "these  may 
be  of  great  concernment  in  reference  to  the  common 
doctrine  of  the  necessity  of  unceasing  influence  from 
the  brain,  being  so  requisite  to  sense  and  motion." 
Boyle's  curiosity  about  the  i^rain  and  its  workings  was 
interwoven  with  his  great  interest  in  theology,  al- 
though his  views  on  the  latter  did  not  please  the 
theologians.  Dean  Swift  was  even  moved  to  parody 
them  in  a  satire  called  A  Pious  Meditation  upon  a  Broom- 
stick in  the  Slv/e  of  t/ie  Honourable  Mr.  Boyle. 

Glis.son  (62)  had  also  distinguished  between  'willed' 
mo\ements  and  those  of  decapitated  animals.  He 
thought  the  latter  analogous  to  a  class  of  movements 
depending  on  a  lower  form  of  perception  not  reaching 
the  mind.  One  might  become  aware  of  them  (^perceptio 
sensitiva)  but  the\'  were  not  ruled  by  the  mind  as  were 
\'oluntarv  mo\'ements  Qierceplio  perceptioms^. 

Whytt's  experiments  (183)  carried  the  argument 
farther  for  he  showed  that  this  type  of  in\oluntary 
motion  could  not  be  explained  as  due  to  the  innate 
irritability  (jf  muscle  tissue  (Haller's  vis  insita~),  for 
preservation  of  the  spinal  marrow  was  essential  for  it. 
He  was,  however,  not  the  first  to  discover  that  the 
spinal  cord  was  essential  for  this  type  of  movement. 
He  had  been  anticipated  by  the  Reverend  Stephen 
Hales,  whose  many  and  brilliant  physiological  experi- 
ments make  one  wonder  how^  much  time  he  gave  to 
his  parishioners  in  Teddington.  Whytt  gives  full  credit 
to  Hales,  for  he  says,  "The  late  reverend  and  learned 
Dr.  Hales  informed  me  that  having  many  years  since 
tied  a  ligature  about  the  neck  of  a  frog  to  prevent  any 
effusion  of  blood,  he  cut  ofT  its  head  ...  the  frog  also 
at  this  time  moved  its  body  when  stimulated,  but  that 
on  thrusting  a  needle  down  the  spinal  marrow,  the 
animal  was  strongly  convulsed  and  immediately  after 
became  motionless."  Alexander  Stuart  (184)  repeated 

i8a.  Boyle,  Robert  (1627-1691).  Considerations  touching  on  the 
Usefulness  of  Experimental  Natural  Philosophy.  London, 
,663. 

183.  VVh^tt,  R.  Observations  on  the  .Xalure,  Causes  and  Cure  of 
those  Disorders  which  are  commonly  called  .\ervous.  Hypo- 
chondriac, or  Hysteric,  to  which  are  prefixed  some  remarks  on  the 
sympathy  of  the  nerves.  Edinburgh :  Balfour,  1 765. 

184.  Stuart,  A.  Three  lectures  on  muscular  motion,  read  before 
the  Royal  Society  in  the  year  MDCCXXXVHI.  London: 
Woodward,  1739. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY  33 


^r<u>.3  ■ 


FIG.  ii.  Lejt:  Alexander  Stuart's  experiment  contirming  the  observations  of  Stephen  Hales  that  a 
decapitated  frog  convulses  on  being  pithed  and  then  becomes  immobile.  (From  Stuart,  A.  Crooman 
Lectures  1738.  London:  Woodward,  1739.)  Ri^ht:  Robert  Whytt  whose  experiments  demonstrated 
reflex  action  in  decapitated  animals  and  the  eflTects  of  spinal  shock.  (From  the  portrait  in  the  Royal 
College  of  Physicians,  Edinburgh,  by  courtesy  of  Mr.  G.  R.  Pendrill.) 


and  confirmed  this  experiment  and  described  it  in  a 
lecture  to  the  Royal  Society  in  1738. 

Whytt  in  his  experiments  on  the  frog  came  very 
close  to  defining  the  segmental  reflex.  He  also  noted 
spinal  shock,  for  he  remarked  that  a  decapitated  frog 
could  not  be  made  to  move  immediately  after  transec- 
tion although  if  one  waited  about  15  min.  it  would 
react  to  stimuli.  But  perhaps  the  most  striking  of  his 
observations  is  the  one  in  which  he  anticipated 
Sherrington  in  regard  to  the  stretch  reflex.  "Whatever 
stretches  the  fibres  of  any  muscle  so  far  as  to  extend 
them  beyond  their  u.sual  length,  excites  them  into 
contraction  about  in  the  same  manner  as  if  they  had 
been  irritated  by  any  sharp  instrument,  or  acrid 
liquor"  (183,  p.  9). 

With  the  publication  of  Whytt's  work  physiologists 
were  divided  between  regarding  the  movements  of 
spinal  animals  as  a  lingering  in  the  cord  of  powers 
originally  derived  from  the  brain,  and  the  view  that 
the  spinal  marrow  itself  was  capable  of  sensation  and 
movement.  Whytt  inclined  to  the  latter  view  in  his 
explanation  of  the  writhings  of  decapitated  and 
eviscerated  snakes.  "We  are  naturally  led  to  con- 
clude," he  said,  "that  they  are  still  in  some  sense  alive, 
and  endued  with  feeling,  i.e.  animated  by  a  sentient 
principle." 

Before  the  end  of  the  century,  Whytt's  publications 
had  been  followed  by  thase  of  Unzer  (185),  of  Halle 

185.   Unzer,  Johann   August  (i  727-1809).   Ersle  Griinde  einer 
Physiologie  der  eigenltchten  thierischen  Nairn  thicrischer  hovper 


and  of  his  pupil  Proehaska  (186)  who  was  a  practising 
ophthalmologist  in  Prague.  Both  these  men  contrib- 
uted more  in  systematization  and  formulation  at  the 
conceptual  level  than  in  the  addition  of  new  experi- 
mental facts.  In  England,  the  Sydenham  Society  gave 
Ijoth  their  books  to  the  same  translator,  Thomas 
Laycock  (the  teacher  of  Hughlings  Jackson),  and 
through  him  the  word  reflexion  became  the  accepted 
term.  Unzer  postulated  several  sites  where  reflexion 
of  impressions  might  take  place — in  the  brain,  in  the 
ganglia,  in  bifurcations  of  nerves  and  in  plexuses.  Only 
if  they  reached  the  brain  would  these  impressions  be 
consciously  perceived.  Unzer  in  discussing  automatic 
movements  protected  himself  against  the  attacks  en- 
countered by  soine  of  his  predecessors  by  saying  that 
"the  animal  machines  are  mysteriously  and  inscru- 
tably endowed  by  the  Creator." 

Proehaska,  with  one  foot  in  the  past,  believed  in  a 
sensorium  commune  where  automatic  reflexion  took 
place  and  thought  this  might  be  in  the  medulla  or  the 
cord  but  did  not  agree  with  Unzer  that  reflexion 
might  be  at  the  level  of  the  ganglia.  He  reverted  to 


Leipzig:  Wiedmanns,  1771;  English  translation  by  T. 
Laycock.  Principles  of  a  Physiology  of  the  Mature  of  Animal 
Organisms.  London:  Sydenham  Society,  1851. 
186.  Prochaska,  JiRi  (1749-1820).  Part  III:  De  functionibus 
systematis  nervosi,  et  observationes  anatomico-pathologi- 
cae.  In:  Adnotationum  Academicarum .  Prague:  Gerle,  1784; 
English  translation  by  T.  Laycock.  Dissertation  on  the 
Functions  of  the  Nervous  System.  London:  Sydenham  Society, 
1851. 


34 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Afinlifsts 

of 

Tlir  Iliustiillir  iVu.,u.s  Sj/slem. 


FIG.  22.  Lffl.  Marshall  Hall.  Right:  one  of  his  experiments 
to  demonstrate  the  three  parts  of  the  reflex  arc.  The  arc  was 
broken  by  any  of  the  following  procedures:  a)  skinning  the 
extremity  (at  5)  (the  'esodic'  nerves);  A)  sectioning  of  the 
"brachial  or  the  lumbar  or  femoral  nerve  leading  to  the  point 
irritated"  (i.e.  the  exodic  nerve'  at  2);  or  c')  removing  the  spinal 
mcirrow  (the  spinal  centre')-  (From  Hall,  M.  Synopsis  of  the 
Diastalttc  J^'ervous  System:  being  outlines  of  the  Croonian  Lec- 
tures delivered  at  the  Royal  College  of  Physicians  in  April 
1850.) 


the  idea  of  an  inherent  vis  nervosa  in  the  nerves  that 
enabled  them  to  function  in  isolation  from  the  brain 
and  he  supported  this  argument  by  citintr  the  move- 
ments of  anencephalic  monsters.  In  his  view  the 
■purpose'  of  reflex  activity  was  preservation  of  the 
individual. 

Here  the  history  of  reflex  activity  rested  for  nearly 
30  years  and  the  next  advance  was  a  technical  rather 
than  a  conceptual  one.  This  was  the  perfection  by 
Legallois  (187)  of  a  method  for  the  artificial  respira- 
tion of  mainmals  and  from  then  on,  in  many  labora- 
tories, heads  began  to  fall.  Legallois,  by  sectioning  the 
neuraxis  serially  from  above  and  from  below,  nar- 
rowed the  center  of  activity  drastically  and  was  so 
impressed  by  the  amount  of  sensorimotor  function 
left  in  a  segment  that  he  rather  sweepingly  concluded 
that  the  spinal  cord  was  the  principal  seat  of  sensation 
and  the  source  of  voluntary  motion.  Although  this 
extreme  view  did  not  gather  many  adherents,  it  was 
clear  that  the  spinal  cord  could  no  longer  be  thought 
of  as  a  mere  prolongation  and  bundling  together  of 
peripheral  nerves.  On  the  contrary,  the  tendency  now 

187.  Legallois,  Julien  Jean  Cesar  (1770-1814).  Experiences 
sur  la  principe  de  la  vie,  notamment  stir  celui  des  mouvemenls 
du  coeur,  el  sur  le  siege  de  ce  principe.  Paris:  D'Hautel,  1812. 


was  to  regard  it  as  a  caudal  extension  of  the  brain. 
Legallois  should  be  remembered  for  being  the  first  to 
recognize  clearly  that  the  respiratory  center  lay  in  the 
medulla  oljlongata. 

This  was  the  setting  of  the  stage  for  the  man  who 
lifted  the  whole  subject  of  reflex  activity  into  the 
framework  of  modern  neurophysiology  and  into 
clinical  science.  Marshall  Hall,  an  Englishmen  edu- 
cated in  the  great  school  at  Edinburgh  where  he  was 
a  pupil  of  the  third  Monro,  was  a  successful  practising 
physician  who  set  up  a  laboratory  in  his  own  house 
(in  Malet  Street  where  the  present  buildings  of 
London  L'niversity  stand).  Here  he  worked  on  his 
animals,  mostly  frogs  and  reptiles,  collating  his  obser- 
vations (188)  with  those  he  made  on  patients  (189). 
His  acumen  enabled  him  to  perceive  several  details 
that  had  escaped  his  predecessors.  For  example,  the 
writhings  of  the  decapitated  snake  that  had  led  Whytt 
to  a  postulate  of  lingering  'life'  within  the  cord  were 
recognized  by  Hall  as  motor  responses  to  the  renewed 
sensory  stimuli  set  up  by  each  movement 

Like  Unzer,  Hall  in  his  work  on  the  machine-like 
movements  of  decapitated  animals  protected  himself 
from  onslaught  by  stating  them  to  be  "all  beautiful 
and  demonstrative  of  the  wisdom  of  Him  who  fashion- 
eth  all  things  after  his  own  Will."  Hall,  again  like 
LTnzer,  realized  that  the  sensory  impression  that  set 
ofl'  a  reflex  need  not  be  consciously  perceived,  al- 
though he  was  consistently  remiss  in  acknowledging 
the  contributions  of  his  predecessors.  He  also  ignored 
the  work  of  his  contemporaries,  for  nowhere  does  he 
refer  to  the  great  blossoming  of  knowledge  of  nerve 
physiology  that  was  taking  place  at  this  time  and 
which  has  been  reviewed  in  an  earlier  section  of  this 
essay.  He  seems  also  to  have  ijeen  unaware  of  the  con- 
tractility of  involuntary  muscle  although  Baglivi 
(190)  over  a  hundred  years  before  he  had  made  the 
distinction  between  smooth  and  striated  muscle.  Hall 
had  many  detractors  who  vigorously  accused  him  of 
plagiarism,  both  from  Miiller  and  from  Prochaska. 
The  first  challenge  was  easier  to  meet  than  the  second, 
for  Hall's  earliest  communication  (191)  antedated 
Miiller's  publication  (94)  on  decapitated  animals  by 
one  year.  In  the  published  report  of  this  first  paper, 

188.  Hall,  Marshall  (1790-1857).  .\'ew  Memoir  on  the  Nerv- 
ous System.  London,  1843. 

189.  Hall,  M.  Diseases  and  Derangements  of  the  Nervous  System. 
London:  Bailliere,  1 841. 

190.  Baglivi,     Giorgio     (1668-1707).     Opera    omnia     medico- 
praclica  et  anatomica.  Leyden:  Anisson  &  Posuel,  1704. 

191.  Hall,  M.  On  a  particular  function  of  the  nervous  system. 
Proc.  Z^ol-  •^O'^-  part  2,  p.  189,  Nov.  27,  1832. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


35 


which  Hall  gave  to  the  Zoological  Society  of  London 
in  November  1832,  there  is,  however,  no  full  descrip- 
tion of  the  reflex  arc  nor  does  he  use  these  terms.  The 
emphasis  is  on  "a  function  of  the  nervous  system  .  .  . 
distinct  from  sensation  and  \oIuntary  or  instinctive 
motion,"  being  a  "property  which  attaches  itself  to 
any  part  of  an  animal,  the  corresponding  portion  of 
the  brain  and  spinal  marrow  of  which  is  entire." 

The  attack  was  pursued  by  others-"  with  great 
bitterness  and  its  leaders  engaged  in  such  unworthy 
acts  as  checking  on  library  slips  to  prove  that  Hall  had 
borrowed  Prochaska's  book.  (The  slips  however  post- 
dated Hall's  original  publications.)  To  the  modern 
worker  the  battle  seems  puerile  and  undignified  and 
one  regrets  that  its  protagonists  did  not  spend  the 
time  on  experiment  instead  of  polemics.-'  Of  the  men 
for  whom  priority  was  being  claimed,  Prochaska  was 
dead  and  it  is  noticeable  that  Miiller,  a  truly  gieat 
man,  after  making  generous  acknowledgement  to 
Hall  in  his  Handbuch  stood  aloof  from  these  bicker- 
ings. 

In  essence  Marshall  Hall's  inajor  contributions  to 
neurophysiology  were,  first  (192),  that  sensory  impres- 
sions coming  into  the  medulla  spinalis  had  far  reach- 
ing effects  in  the  nervous  system  in  addition  to  the 
segmental  efTector  response,'"  secondly  the  recognition 
that  although  reflex  activity  took  place  at  a  spinal 
level  it  could  be  influenced  by  the  wilP^  and  thirdly, 
the  relationship  of  this  fact  to  the  exaggeration  of 
reflex  response  on  removal  of  the  brain  (193).  These 
are  not  the  only  areas  in  which  he  anticipated 
Sherrington.  He  gave  a  preliminary  glimpse  of  the 
stepping  reflex,  "In  the  actions  of  walking  in  man,  I 
iinagine  the  reflex  function  to  play  a  very  considerable 
part,  although  there  are,  doubtless  facts  which 
demonstrate  that  the  contact  of  the  sole  with  the 
ground  is  not  unattended  by  a  certain  influence  upon 
the  action  of  certain  muscles." 

Marshall  Hall  introduced  the  word  'arc'  to  describe 
the  refle.x  pathway.  Many  of  his  other  terms  have, 
happily,  not  been  retained  by  physiologists,  for  he  was 
a  great  lover  of  neologisms,  as  his  definition  of  the 
arc  shows:  "the  existence  in  Anatomy  and  Physiology,  of 
a  continuous  Diastaltic  Nervous  Arc  including  an 
Esodic  Nerve,   the  Spinal  Centre  and   Exodic  Nerve  in 

192.  Hall,  M.  On  the  reflex  function  of  the  medulla  oblongata 
and  medulla  spinalis.  Phil.  Trans.  123:  635,  1833. 

193.  Hall,  M.  On  the  true  spinal  marrow,  and  on  the  excito-motory 
system  of  the  nerves.  Lectures  given  before  the  Royal  So- 
ciety, privately  printed,  1837. 


essential  relation  and  connection  with  each  other — 
and  of  a  series  of  such  Arcs.  .  .  ."  (194).  (One  recog- 
nizes here  that  Queen  Victoria  had  a  rival  among  her 
subjects  in  the  use  of  italics.) 

One  further  contribution  of  Hall's  at  the  conceptual 
level  should  be  noted.  Implicit,  if  not  explicit,  in  the 
theories  of  the  earlier  physiologists  was  the  notion 
that  in  voluntary  movement  volition  directed  a 
nervous  influence  towards  the  individually  appropri- 
ate iTiuscles.  Hall  pointed  out  that  the  will  was  inore 
teleological  and  less  specific  in  its  action  and  not 
"directed  to  any  muscle  or  set  of  muscles,  but  to  an 
aim,  object  and  purpose  of  their  contraction"  (195). 
Hall's  contributions  were  not  evaluated  as  highly  by 
his  contemporaries  as  they  have  been  by  later  physi- 
ologists, though  he  himself  had  no  doubts  as  to  how 
they  should  be  ranked;  he  stated  that  they  were  the 
greatest  advance  in  medical  science  since  William 
Harvey. 

The  iinpact  of  the  work  of  the  physiologists  on  the 
concepts  of  the  psychologists  was  very  great  and  so 
disturbing  that  their  literature  was  filled  with  contro- 
versy for  many  years.  Long  before  the  concept  of 
reflex  acti\ity  was  carried  into  the  brain  by  Sechenov 
to  explain  its  higher  functions,  the  psychologists  were 
in  distress  over  the  implication  for  'sensation,'  for 
'consciousness'  and  for  'volition,'  of  the  developing 
knowledge  of  spinal  reflexes.  The  most  conspicuous 
controversy  was  that  waged  between  Eduard  Pfliiger 
(196),  von  Helmholtz's  successor  at  the  Physiological 

194.  H.'VLL,  M.  Synopsis  oj  the  diastaltic  nervous  system.  Crocnian 
Lectures,  London,  1850. 

195.  Hall,  M.  Memoirs  on  the  Nervous  System.  London,  1837. 

196.  Pfluger,  Edouard  (1829-1910).  Die  sensorischen  Func- 
tionen  des  RUckenmarks  der  Wirbelthiere  nebsi  einer  neuen 
Lehre  iiber  die  Leitungsgesetze  der  Reflexionen.  Berlin,  1853. 


''"Such  as,  for  example,  George,  J.  D.  Contribution  to  the 
history  of  the  nervous  system.  Lond.  med.  Gaz-  22:  40,  93, 
1837-1838. 

-'  A  full  account  of  the  controversy  (though  scarcely  an 
unbiased  one)  can  be  found  in  Longet,  F.  A.  Traite  d'Anatomie 
de  Physiologie  du  Systeme  Nerveiix  de  I'Homme  et  des  Animaux 
Vertehres.  Paris,  1842.  2  vol. 

''-  "But  the  operation  of  the  reflex  function  is  by  no  means 
confined  to  parts  corresponding  to  distinct  portions  of  the 
medulla.  The  irritation  of  a  given  part  may,  on  the  contrary, 
induce  contraction  in  a  part  very  remote."  Phil.  Trans.  123: 
635,  1833. 

''  "The  true  spinal  system  is  susceptible  of  modification  by 
volition.  .  .  ."  Memoirs  on  the  .Nervous  System.  London,  1837, 
part  2,  p.  73.  (This  part  of  the  observation  was  anticipated 
bv  Whvtt.) 


36 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Institute  at  Bonn,  tmd  Rudolph  Lotze  (197),  pro- 
fessor of  Philosophy  at  Gotlingen.  Back  and  forth  the 
battle  raged,  swinging  from  physiology  into  meta- 
physics and  back  again  into  experiment.  The  argu- 
ments all  centered  around  the  problems  of  whether  a 
spinal  animal  was  sentient  and  conscious,  and  whether 
its  movements  were  purposeful.  Was  such  an  animal 
intelligent?  Did  it  have  memory?  Pfliiger  espoused  the 
idea  of  consciousness  in  the  cord,  Lotze  denied  it; 
both  were  dogmatic  inn  to  neither  can  we  look  for 
advancement  of  knowledge  of  the  central  nervous 
system  in  this  context. 

In  the  nineteenth  century,  while  Marshall  Hall  was 
still  alive,  the  nature  of  inhiiiition  became  of  major 
interest  to  physiologists  and  before  the  end  of  the 
century  was  to  have  its  role  in  reflex  activity  demon- 
strated by  Sherrington.  Although  the  po.ssibility  of 
inhibition  had  been  suggested  by  several  workers,  the 
actual  phenomenon  had  first  been  observed  (and  re- 
jected as  an  error  of  experiment)  by  Volkmann  (198) 
in  1838  in  relation  to  the  action  of  the  vagus  on  the 
heart.  It  was  again  observed,  and  this  time  accepted, 
by  the  Weber  brothers  (199)  in  1845.  The  elder 
brother,  Ernst,  held  the  joint  chair  of  anatomy  and 
physiology  at  Leipzig  until  Carl  Ludwig  came  in  1866 
to  take  over  the  latter  section  and  set  up  his  famous 
institute.  The  technique  of  the  classic  experiment  that 
established  the  existence  of  vagal  inhibition  was  the 
stimulation  by  a  voltaic  pile  of  both  vagi  of  the  frog. 
Later  the  Webers  found  that  unilateral  stimulation 
had  the  same  effect  and  they  confirmed  the  result  by 
stimulating  the  vagus  of  a  cat  with  an  induction  cur- 
rent. They  reported  this  discovery,  one  of  the  land- 
marks of  nerve  physiology,  at  the  Congress  of  Italian 
Scientists  held  in  Naples  in  1845  (which  accounts  for 
their  publication  being  in  Latin  rather  than  in 
German).  This  type  of  inhibition,  like  that  which  was 
eventually  evoked  to  explain  Bernard's  (200)  obser- 
vation of  the  influence  of  the  chorda  tympani  on  the 

197.  Lotze,  Rudolph  Heinrich  (1817-1881).  Instinct.  In:  R. 
Wagner.  Handwortrnbuch.  pt.  ■!.  Brunswick:  Vieweg,  184.!- 

1853- 

ig8.  Volkmann,  Alfred  Wilhelm  (1800-1871).  Uber  Re- 
flexbewegungen.  Arch.  Anat.  u.  Physiol.  15,  1838. 

igg.  Weber,  Eduard  Friedrich  Wilhelm  (1806-1871)  and 
Ernst  Heinrich  Weber  (i  795-1878).  Experimenta, 
quibus  probatur  nervos  vagos  rotations  machinae  gal- 
vano-magneticae  irritatos,  motum  cordi  retardare  et 
adeo  intercipare.  Ann.  Univ.  Med..,  Milano  20:  227,  1845. 

200.  Bernard,  Claude  (1813-1878).  Recherches  anatomiques 
et  physiologiques  sur  la  corde  du  tympan,  pour  servir  a 
I'histoire  de  I'hemiplegie  faciale.  Ann.  med.-psychol.  i  :  408, 
'843- 


submaxillary  blood  \'essels,  seemed  simple  to  later 
physiologists  faced  with  the  complexities  of  inhibition 
in  the  central  nervous  system.  These  had  to  await 
exploration  by  Sherrington. 

An  enduring  interest  of  Sherrington  and  one  ex- 
haustively explored  by  him  in  the  laboratory  was  re- 
ciprocal inner\ation  of  antagonist  muscles,  and  many 
of  his  publications  were  on  this  subject.  The  attempt 
of  Descartes  (25)  in  the  seventeenth  century  to  reach 
an  explanation  based  on  channeling  of  vital  spirits 
had  no  immediate  successor.  In  the  early  part  of  the 
nineteenth  century  Charles  Bell  (201)  had  postulated 
the  existence  of  peripheral  inhibition  by  insisting  on 
the  need  for  nerves  which  had  the  opposite  of  an 
excitatory  effect  on  muscle.  "The  nerves,"  he  said, 
"have  been  considered  so  generally  as  in.struments  for 
stimulating  the  muscles,  without  thought  of  their  act- 
ing in  the  opposite  capacity,  that  some  additional 
illustration  may  be  necessary."  He  went  on  to  describe 
an  experiment  in  which  contraction  of  a  flexor  muscle 
coincided  with  imposed  relaxation  of  its  opponent 
extensor. 

The  possibility  of  a  peripherally  exerted  inhiljition 
of  muscle  contractility  attracted  many  people  at 
about  this  time.  One  of  the  earliest  was  a  Dr.  West 
(202)  of  Alford  in  Lincolnshire  (who  had  heard  Bell's 
lectures  at  the  Royal  College  of  Surgeons).  \Vest's 
suggestion  was  that  contraction  was  an  inherent  prop- 
erty of  muscle  and  that  the  action  of  the  nerve  supply- 
ing it  was  not  to  evoke,  but  to  'restrain'  or  'rein'  this 
innate  tendency  to  contract.  He  explained  a  volun- 
tary contraction  as  a  withdrawal  of  this  nervous  re- 
straint "so  as  to  allow  the  peculiar  property  of  muscu- 
lar fibre  to  shew  itself."  The  publication  of  West's 
hypothesis  provoked  some  expostulation,  one  anony- 
mous correspondent  saying  this  was  "certainly  one  of 
the  clumsiest  contrivances  that  nature  was  ever 
accused  of"  The  mechanism  of  rigor  mortis  was  not 
understood  at  this  time  and  West  felt  that  his  theory 
off"ered  a  possible  explanation.  The  idea  was  also 
present  in  the  arguments  of  many  others,  for  example 
those  of  Engel  (203),  of  Stannius  (204)  and  of  Duges 

201.  Bell,  C.  On  tiio  ner\es  of  the  orbit.  Phil.  Trans.  113:  289, 
1823. 

202.  West,  R.  Uvedale.  On  tlic  inHucnce  of  the  nerves  over 
muscular  contractility.  Ryan's  Med.  Surg.  J.  1  :  24,  245, 
445,  1832. 

203.  Engel,  Joseph.  Uber  Muskelreizbarkeit.  ^Ischr.  Gesellsch. 
Arize,  yVien  i  :  205,  252,  1849. 

204.  Stannius,  Hermann  (1808-1883).  Untcrsuchungen  iiber 
die  Leistungsfahigheit  der  Muskeln  u.  Todtenstarre. 
Vierordt's  Arch,  physwl.  heilk.  i,  1852. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


37 


f).    OO    If! 


Tiglira  Murculi  [eamdum  auto-  "''■'"'■  ^-^ 


FIG.  23.  Lc//.-  Descartes'  sketch  of  reciprocal  muscles  of  the  eye  (/Jf  Humine,  the  Latin  translation  by 
Schuyl).  Center:  a  redrawing  showing  closure  of  valves  on  relaxation,  opening  on  contraction  to  allow 
animal  spirits  to  How  in  and  swell  the  muscle  QL' Homme,  the  French  edition  of  1677).  Right:  Sherring- 
ton's diagram  of  the  connections  and  actions  of  two  cells  of  a  dorsal  root  ganglion.  The  plus  sign 
indicates  that  at  the  central  synapses  the  afferent  impulses  excite  the  ipsilateral  flexor  muscle  and 
the  contralateral  extensor,  while  inhibiting  the  ipsilateral  extensor  and  the  contralateral  flexor 
muscle.  (From  Sherrington,  C.  S.  The  Integratwe  Ac/inn  nf  Ike  Nervous  Svslem,  2nd  ed.  Cambridge: 
Cambridge,  1947.) 


(205)  in  Montpellier.  The  latter  favored  a  peripherally 
exerted  nervous  influence  acting  against  an  inherent 
elasticity  of  muscle. 

In  1868  Hering  (206)  and  Breuer(207)  found  in  the 
respiratory  system  a  parallel  to  Bell's  experiment 
whereby  distention  of  the  lung  acting  through  the 
pulmonary  branch  of  the  vagus  inhibited  inspiration 
while  exciting  expiration,  the  well-known  Hering- 
Breuer  reflex.  And  in  1883  Kronecker  (208)  working 
on  the  swallowing  reflex  in  Ludwig's  laboratory  with 
his  American  pupil,  Meltzer,  demonstrated  the  in- 
hibitory action  of  the  superior  laryngeal  nerve  on  in- 
spiratory muscles  during  contraction  of  expiratory 
ones.  The  reflex  nature  of  .swallowing  had  been  recog- 

205.  Duces,  Antoine.  Traile  de  Physiologie  Comparee  de  I'homme 
et  des  Animaux.  Montpellier  &  Paris,  1838;  Compt.  rend.  Sac. 
de  biol.  March  17,  1847. 

206.  Hering,  Karl  Ewald  Konstantin  (i  834-1918).  Die 
Selbststeuerung  der  Athmung  durch  den  Nervus  Vagus. 
Silzber.  Akad.  Wiss.  Wien  57;  672,  1868. 

207.  Breuer,  Joseph  (1842-1925).  Die  Selbstseuerung  der 
Athmung  durch  den  Nervus  Vagus.  Sitzber.  Akad.  Wiss, 
Wien  58:  909,  1868. 

208.  Kronecker,  Karl  Hugo  (1839-1914)  and  Samuel 
James  Meltzer  (1851-1920).  Der  Schluckmechanismus, 
seine  Erregung  und  seine  Hemmung.  Arch.  Anat.  Physiol. 
Suppl. :  328,  1883. 


nized  by  Marshall  Hall  (195)  in  1823  and  the  direct 
afferent  nerve  for  it  had  been  identified  by  Magcndie 
(209)  to  be  the  glossopharyngeal,  but  the  reciprocal 
effect  had  not  been  noted  by  them. 

It  is  the  fact  that  there  are  no  inhibitory  nerves  to 
vertebrate  skeletal  muscle  that  drew  the  whole  subject 
of  reflex  inhibition  into  the  central  nervous  system. 
With  the  realization  that  reflex  inhibition  had  its  site 
in  the  central  nervous  system,  attention  was  turned  to 
the  connection  between  the  incoming  sensory  element 
of  the  arc  and  the  motor  component,  to  the  junction 
between  them,  in  other  words,  to  the  synapse  (Sher- 
rington's word).  That  there  might  be  an  interaction  of 
a  synaptic  kind  between  neurons  in  the  periphery  had 
occurred  to  several  workers,  one  among  whom  was 
Sigmund  Freud  (210).  His  work  on  fresh-water  crabs 
and  his  illustrative  sketches  of  how  he  conceived  of 
intercommunication  between  the  axons  of  their 
ganglia  came  close  to  what  is  now  termed  an  ephapse, 
although  he  pictured  transverse  crossings  that  sug- 
gest a  uniting  of  fibers  rather  than  a  contiguity. 

209.  Magendie,  F.  Lei^ons  sitr  les  fonctions  du  systhne  nerveux. 
Paris,  1839. 

210.  Freud,  Sigmund  (1856- 1939).  Uber  den  Bau  der  Nerven- 
fasern  und  Nervenzellen  beim  Flusskrebs.  Sitzber.  Akad. 
Wiss.   Wien  85:  9,  1882. 


38 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


.<V.    DIAGRAMS  ILLCSTHATrNG  THE  ELEMENTARY 
COMBINATIONS  OF  THE  NERVOUS  SYSTEM. 


r    A 


FIG.  24.  Above:  Schema  of  the  connections  between  the  posterior  and  an- 
terior roots  of  the  spinal  cord  as  taught  to  students  in  the  days  before  the 
neuron  doctrine  and  the  theory  of  the  synapse.  [From  Bernard,  C.  Lemons 
sur  la  Physiologie  el  la  Palhologie  du  Sysleme  .Nerveux.  Paris:  Balliere,  1858.) 
Right:  Connections  in  the  nervous  system  as  taught  to  students  in  1885. 
(From  Pye-Smith,  P.  H.  Syllabus  of  a  course  of  lectures  on  Physiology  delivered  at 
Guy's  Hospital.  London:  Churchill,  1885.) 


Recognition  of  the  synapse  could  come  only  after 
the  neuron  theory  had  replaced  the  reticular  theory. 
According  to  the  latter,  strongly  championed  by 
von  Gerlach  (211),  nerve  cells  were  connected  with 
each  other  by  a  diffuse  fibrillary  network  forming  an 
anastomosis.  This  hypothesis  received  support  from 
Golgi  (212),  although  it  was  his  silver  staining  tech- 
nique in  the  hands  of  Ramon  y  Cajal  (213)  that 
finally  disproved  it,  for  Ramon  y  Cajal  established 
that  both  axons  and  dendrites  had  free  endings.  To- 
gether they  shared  the  Nobel  prize  in  igo6,  Golgi 
devoting  his  address  to  an  attack  on  the  neuron  theory 
that  his  fellow  prize  winner  had  done  so  much  to  up- 
hold. In  modern  times,  the  synapse  (an  abstraction) 
is  having  to  be  remodelled  in  the  light  of  what  the 
electron  microscope  is  revealing. 

The  nature  of  central  inhibition,  a  still  incompletely 

211.  VON  Gerlach,  Joseph  (1820- 1896).  The  spinal  cord.  In: 
S.  Strieker,  A  Aianual  of  Histology  (English  translation). 
London:  New  Sydenham  Society,  1872. 

212.  Golgi,  Camillo  (1844-1926).  Atti  Soc.  ital.  progr.  sc. 
3rd  reunion.  1910. 

213.  Ramon  y  Caj.\l,  Santiago  (1852- 1934).  Neuron  theory 
or  reticular  theory.  Arch.  Jisiol.  5,  igo8;  translation  by 
Purkiss  and  Fox.  Madrid,  1954. 


resolved  issue,  has  e\oked  many  hypotheses.  Among 
them,  those  depending  on  mutual  interference  of 
impulses  at  the  effector  component  of  the  reflex  arc 
form  one  class.  An  example  is  the  schema  suggested 
by  Rosenthal  (214)  in  1862  to  explain  the  effect  of 
efferent  vagus  fibers  on  the  respiratory  center.  He 
proposed  that  an  effector  system  excited  into  action 
by  one  nerve  could  have  the  pulsating  rhythm  of  its 
nervous  supply  disturbed  by  inflow  from  another 
nerve,  the  result  being  a  redistribution  of  previously 
grouped  impulses  into  more  frequent  but  less  powerful 
(and  hence  inadequate)  discharges.  Lack  of  evidence 
for  a  pulse-like  time-rhythm  in  nerve  trunks  led  to  the 
rejection  of  this  hypothesis  by  W'undt,  Sherrington 
and  others. 

In  the  1870's  and  1880's  attempts  to  explain  inhi- 
bition on  metabolic  effects  depending  directly  on  the 
cell's  response  to  stimulation  being  an  assimilation  of 
chemical  nutrients  were  espoused   by  Gaskell  (215) 

214.  Rosenthal,  Joseph.  Die  Atembeweg  und  ihre  Bezichung  zum 
nervus  Vagus.  Berlin,  1862. 

215.  Gaskell,  Walter  Holbrook  (1847-1914).  On  the 
rhythm  of  the  heart  of  the  frog  and  of  the  nature  of  the 
action  of  the  vagus  nerve    Phil.  Trans.  173;  993,  1882. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


39 


(for  the  vagus)  and  Hering  (216)  (for  black-white 
sensations  of  the  visual  sense),  by  Vervvorn  (2 1  7)  (in 
his  Biogenhvpothese).  The  hypothesis  did  not  survive 
for  long.  As  Forbes  (218)  said  in  his  critique,  "To 
assume  that  increase  of  anabolism  necessarily  implies 
decrease  of  catabolism,  is  to  suppose  that  increasing  a 
man's  salary  ensures  decrease  of  his  expenditure."  A 
theory  of  immobilization  of  ion  transfer  during  inhi- 
l)ition  was  propo.sed  by  Macdonald  (219)  in  1905,  at 
a  time  when  the  release  of  potassium  from  injured 
nerves  was  receiving  considerable  attention. 

With  the  discovery  of  the  refractory  period  in  nerve 
[by  Gotch  and  Burch  (220)  in  1889]  there  was  some 
tendency  to  regard  block  of  conduction  due  to  excita- 
tory impulses  arriving  during  refractoriness  caused  by 
preceding  excitation  to  be  the  mechanism  of  inhibi- 
tion. This  is  now  recognized  as  a  misuse  of  the  term, 
and  in  fact  Sherrington's  demonstration  that  after 
discharge  persisting  after  cessation  of  excitation  could 
be  cut  short  by  inhibitory  nerve  action  was  an  early 
salutory  corrective. 

In  the  course  of  researches  on  the  inexhaustibility 
of  nerve,  a  subject  which  engrossed  the  early  electro- 
physiologists,  Wedensky  (221)  found  that  a  rapid 
series  of  strong  stimuli  would  fail  to  produce  more 
than  a  single  twitch  if  the  transmission  from  nerve  to 
muscle  were  blocked  either  by  fatigue  at  the  end  plate 
or  by  artificially  impairing  a  section  of  the  nerve  by 
narcosis.  If  however  the  frequency  or  the  strength  of 
the  tetanus  were  then  reduced,  the  muscle  went  im- 
mediately into  tetanic  contraction.  Wedensky  con- 
cluded that  the  nerve  was  inexhaustible  and  that  the 
phenomenon  was  one  of  inhibition.  This  may,  how- 
ever, be  regarded  as  a  special  usage  of  the  term  since 
the  effect  he  observed  was  merely  a  characteristic  of 
the  relative  refractory  period  of  nerve  and  its  time 
course  as  related  to  strength  of  stimulus  (222). 

It  was  Sherrington's  insistence  on  a  central  site  for 

216.  Hering,  Heinrich  Ewald  (1866-1948).  Zur  Thcorio  dc 
Vorgange  in  der  lebendigen  Substanz.  Lotos  g:  35,  1889; 
translated  in  Brain  20:  232,  1897. 

217.  Verworn,  Max.  Die  Biogmhypothcse.  Jena:  Fischer,  1903. 

218.  Forbes,  Alexander.  Reflex  inhibition  of  skeletal  muscle. 
Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  5:  149,  1912. 

219.  Macdonald,  J.  S.  The  structure  and  function  of  nerve 
fibres.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  76:  322,  1905. 

220.  Gotch,  F.  and  G.  J.  Burch.  The  electrical  response  of 
nerve  to  two  stimuli.  J.  PhysioL  24:  410,  1899. 

221.  Wedensky,  Nicholai  Yevgenevich  (1852- 1922).  Die 
Erregung,  Hemmung  und  Narkose.  .Arch.  s^es.  Physiol. 
100:  I,  1903. 

222.  Adrian,  E.  D.  Wedensky  inhibition  in  relation  to  the 
"all-or-none"  principle  in  nerve.  J.  PhysioL  46:  384,  1913. 


the  inhibitory  mechanisms  of  skeletal  muscle  that 
emphasized  the  reflex  nature  of  inhibition.  The  con- 
tributions of  Sherrington  and  his  school  are  the  basis 
of  modern  ideas  of  the  reflex  at  the  spinal  level.  A 
great  number  of  findings  (223-227)  made  by  Sherring- 
ton and  brought  together  into  a  unifying  explanatory 
scheme  included  the  following  major  observations:  that 
postural  tonus  of  a  muscle  is  dependent  not  only  on 
efferent  nerves  but  on  afferent  nerves  from  that  muscle 
itself,  the  stimulus  to  the  latter  being  from  stretch  re- 
ceptors [the  myotatic  reflex  (223)];  that  decerebrate 
rigidity  (224)  is  an  e.xaggerated  muscle  tonus  in  the 
antigravity  muscles — a  reflex  standing  ["an  harmo- 
nious congerie  of  stretch-reflexes"  (225)];  that  the 
afferent  nerve  from  a  given  muscle  can  elicit  a  con- 
traction in  that  muscle  itself  (228),  without  involve- 
ment of  the  opposing  muscles  of  the  joint;-''  that  the 
main  stimulus  for  the  stepping  reflex  (229)  does  not 
come  from  contact  of  the  foot  with  ground,  as  might 
be  expected;-"  that  stimulation  causing  fle.xion  in  one 

223.  LiDDELL,    E.    G.    T.    AND    C.    S.    SHERRINGTON.    ReflcXCS    in 

response   to   stretch   (myotatic   reflexes).    Proc.    Roy.   Soc, 
London,  ser.  fi  96:  212,  1924. 

224.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Cateleptoid  reflexes  in  the  monkey. 
Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  60:  41  I,  1897. 

225.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Problems  of  muscular  receptivity. 
Linacre  Lecture.  Mature,  London  113:  732,  892,  929,  1924. 

226.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Selected  Writings  of  C.  S.  Sherrington, 
edited  by  D.  Denny-Brown.  London:  Hamish  Hamilton, 
1940. 

227.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Note  on  the  knee-jerk  and  the  corre- 
lation of  action  of  antagonistic  muscles.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc, 
London,  ser.  B  52:  556,  1892-3. 

228.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  On  reciprocal  innervation  of  an- 
tagonistic muscle  (eighth  note).  Proc  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser. 
B  76:  269,  1905. 

229.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Flexion-reflex  of  the  limb,  crossed 
extension-reflex,  and  reflex  stepping  and  standing.  J. 
Physiol.  40:  28,  1 910. 

''*  From  a  series  of  1 4  articles  by  Sherrington  on  reciprocal 
innervation  stretching  over  the  years  from  1893  to  1909  (and 
developed  in  many  other  of  his  writings),  the  following  excerpt 
may  be  quoted  as  one  of  his  crucial  experiments:  "All  the 
nerves  of  the  limb  being  severed,  except  those  of  the  vasti  and 
crureus,  the  animal  is  inverted  and  the  knee  then  gently  but 
fully  extended  by  raising  the  foot,  the  thigh  being  held  vertical. 
The  foot  is  then  released,  the  anticrus  falls,  and  in  doing  so  is 
seen  to  be  suddenly  checked  by  exciting  a  contraction  of  the 
extensor  of  the  knee.  This  contraction  is  different  from  a  knee- 
jerk,  for  it  only  slowly  passes  off."  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Proc. 
Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  76:  283,   1905. 

''  ".  .  .in  the  intact  animal  (cat,  dog),  severance  of  all  the 
nerve  trunks  directly  distributed  to  all  four  of  the  feet  up  to  and 
above  the  wrists  and  ankles  impairs  walking  so  little  £is  to  make 
it  highly  unlikely  that  the  loss  of  receptivity  of  the  feet  destroys 
any  large  factor  in  the  reflex  basis  of  these  acts  '  (235). 


40 


HANDBOOK    OF    Pin'SrOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


In 


K^\    )> 


"/ 


'  'I  '-  y 

Fio.  25.  Charles  Scott  Sherrington,  from  the  drawing  by  Reginald  Eves  (reproduced  by  permis- 
sion from  Selected  Writings  of  Sir  Charin  Sherrington,  edited  by  D.  Denny  Brown.  New  Vorii:  Hoeber, 
1940).  Right:  Sherrington's  classic  picture  of  the  areas  for  the  scratch  reflex  in  the  dog.  (From 
Sherrington,  C.  S.    The  Integrative  Action  of  the  Nervous  System.  Cambridge:  Cambridge,  1947.) 


limb  frequently  evokes  an  exten.sor  movement  in  the 
contralateral  homologous  limb  [the  crossed-extensor 
reflex  (229)];  that  this  reflex  can  also  be  centrally 
inhiljited;  and  that  after  prolonged  inhibitory  stimu- 
lation there  is,  on  withdrawal  of  the  stimulus,  an  in- 
crease of  contraction  ['reflex  reljound'  (230)].  These 
are  only  a  few  of  the  reflex  phenomena  that  received 
elucidation  through  .Sherrington's  work. 

Out  of  a  vast  numljcr  of  laboratory  experiments 
grew  his  unifying  hypothesis  of  reflex  excitation  and 
reflex  inhibition,  and  hence  of  an  interdependence  of 
reflex  arcs  resulting  in  an  integrative  action  of  the 
nervous  system.  .Sherrington's  clas.sic  book  bearing 
this  title  was  published  (231)  when  he  was  Professor 
of  Physiology  at  Liverpool  University  and  was  based 
on  lectures  he  gave  at  Yale  University.  The  concepts 
of  'the  final  common  path,'  of  'synaptic  connections,' 
of  'central  inhilaition,'  of  'central  excitation"  and  of 
'reciprocal  innervation'  are  incorporated  in  modern 
ph\siology  which  recognizes  its  deln  to  Sherrington. 
The  nineteenth  century  which  had  opened  with  only 
one  method  for  tracing  fiber  tracts — that  of  dissecting 
them  out  as  Bichat  had  done — gave  to  physiologists 
two    great    new    tools,    the    histological    method    of 

2'50.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Strychnine  and  reflex  inhibition  of 
skeletal  muscle.  J.  Physiol.  36:  185,  1907. 

231.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  The  Integrative  Action  of  the  .\ervous 
System.  New  York;  Scribners,  1906;  new  edition.  Cam- 
bridge: Cambridge,  1947. 


Wallerian  degeneration  and  the  technique  of  electrical 
recordings.  In  the  hands  of  \'ictor  Horsley  and  his 
associates,  Gotch,  Beever,  Schafer  and  others,  electro- 
physiology  of  spinal-cord  systems  made  great  advances 
which  can  be  followed  in  the  series  of  papers  pub- 
lished in  the  Philiisoji/itcal  Transactions  between 
1886  and  1 89 1.  An  overall  view  of  what  could  be 
achieved  by  this  new  method  is  given  in  the  Croonian 
Lecture  of  Gotch  and  Horsley  in  1891  (232). 

Towards  the  end  of  the  century  these  techniques 
were  being  applied,  not  only  by  Horsley,  but  by  many 
of  his  contemporaries  to  the  study  of  the  physiology 
of  the  brain. 


PHYSIOLOG^■    OF    THE    BR.MN :    DEVELOPMENT    OF 
IDE.'SiS   AND   GROWTH    OF  EXPERIMENT 

At  the  mid-eighteenth  century,  scientists  seeking 
knowledge  of  the  brain  could  look  back  on  a  history 
of  their  field  that  revealed  a  gradual  evolution  of 
anatomical  knowledge  about  its  structure  but  only 
conjecture  about  its  physiology. 

Among  the  early  Greeks  the  teachings  of  Plato  had 
placed  man's  rational  faculties  where  we  would  put 

232.  Gotch,     F.     .\nd     Victor     Horsley.     On     the    mam- 
malian nervous  system,  its  functions  and  their  localization 
determined  by  an  electrical  method.  Phil.   Trims.  B  182 
267,  1 89 1. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


4' 


*j.'~ 


J..'^*— >^    ..«»|o1.lCr'Jv.-'l>*-'"'' 


FIG.  j6.  /,('//•  th<"  tliree  ventricles  of  the  brain  as  ens  isaged  by  Albertus  Magnus.  Right:  Leonardo 
da  Vinci's  wax  cast,  the  first  CNperimental  determination  of  their  shape. 


them  now,  in  the  head;  the  passions  he  put  in  the 
spinal  marrow  relating  them  to  the  heart,  and  the 
lower  appetites  were  given  a  place  in  the  cord  below 
the  diaphragm  where  they  could  play  upon  the  liver. 
For  Plato  these  were  the  divisions  of  man's  tripartite 
soul. 

Under  the  influence  of  Galen  the  spinal  nervous 
system  lost  this  position  of  importance,  for  according 
to  his  doctrine  other  organs  of  the  bod\',  the  liver  and 
the  heart,  were  the  primary  sites  for  manufacture  and 
transmutation  of  the  spirits.  From  the  Islamic  physi- 
cians came  the  emphasis  on  three  ventricles  with 
different  functions,  an  anterior  ventricle  being  the 
receiver  of  all  incoming  spirits,  a  'sensus  communis,' 
whereas  a  posterior  ventricle  formed  the  reservoir  for 
the  outflow  of  animal  spirits  to  all  muscles  through 
their  nerves.  In  a  middle  ventricle  was  to  be  found 
man's  rea.son.  Similar  ideas  about  triple  cavities  in 
the  brain  and  their  allotted  functions  were  generally 
accepted  throughout  the  imenlightencd  middle  ages 
until  finally  an  anatomist,  no  less  a  man  than  Leo- 
nardo da  Vinci  (233),  mapped  the  true  shape  of  the 
ventricles  by  pouring  into  them  melted  wax  to  form 
a  cast. 

Throughout  the  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  cen- 
turies, the  structure  of  the  brain  was  being  unfolded 
by  the  anatomists  but  still  without  a  parallel  investi- 
gation  of  function.    It   was   the   cranial   nerves   that 

■2;j3.  DA  Vinci,  Leonardo  (1452-1519).  On  Ike  Human  Body: 
The  Anatomical,  Physiological,  and  Embryological  Drawings  of 
Leonardo  da  Vinci,  with  translations,  emmendations  and  a 
biographical  introduction  by  C.  D.  O'Malley  and 
J.  B.  deC.  M.  Saunders.  New  York:  Schuman,  1952. 


yielded  first  and  Galen's  seven  pairs-^  (accepted  on 
his  authority  for  1400  years)  swelled  to  nine  in  the 
seventeenth  century.  In  1660  Schneider  (234)  identi- 
fied the  olfactory  pair  and  2  years  later  Willis  (235) 
dis.sected  the  accessory  nerve  that  bears  his  name. 
Today's  recognition  of  1 2  pairs  of  cranial  nerves  dates 
from  the  eighteenth  century  and  the  work  of  von 
Soemmering  (236),  whose  books  are  illustrated  by 
engravings  rivalled  only  by  those  of  Charles  Bell, 
von  Soemmering  wrote  copiously  on  anatomy,  illus- 
trating some  of  his  work  by  his  own  hand  and  some 
by  the  drawings  of  his  pupil  Koeck. 

The  role  played  by  each  pair  of  cranial  nerves  was 
still  in  soine  degree  obscure,  for  some  nerves  appeared 
to  have  more  than  one  function,  and  Whytt  (237) 
was  one  of  the  earlv  workers  to  obserx'e  how  complex 
their  action  might  be.  He  found  that  the  optic  nerve 

234.  Schneider,  Conrad  Victor  (1614-1680).  Liber  primus  de 
catarrhis.  Wittenberg:  Mevius  &  Schumacher,   1660. 

235.  Willis,  Thomas  (1621-1675).  ^^  Anima  Brutorum.  In: 
Opera  Omnia.  Leyden :  Huguetan,  1681. 

236.  Soemmering,  Samuel  Thomas  (1755-1830).  />  basi  en- 
cephali  ei  originibus  nervorum  cranio  egredientum.  Gottingen: 
Vandenhoeck,  1 778. 

237.  Whytt,  Robert  (1714-1766).  An  essay  on  the  vital  and 
other  involuntary  motions  of  animals.  Edinburgh :  Hamilton, 
Balfour  and  Neill,  1751. 


^*  According  to  Galen's  numbering,  the  seven  pairs  of 
cranial  nerves  were:  /)  optic;  2)  oculomotor  and  abducens 
taken  together;  3)  and  4)  were  both  parts  of  what  is  now  called 
the  trigeminal,  j)  facial  together  with  the  auditory;  5)  the 
glossopharyngeal,  vagus  and  accessory  nerves;  7)  the  hypo- 
glossal. 


42 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'H\SIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  27.  Thomas  Willis  and  the  illustration  of  the  base  of 
the  brain  taken  from  his  book  De  cerebri  anatome.  The  circle  of 
Willis,  named  for  him,  had  been  depicted  by  several  anato- 
mists before  him.  Willis  was  fortunate  in  having  Christopher 
Wren  as  his  illustrator. 

was  not  .solely  concerned  with  vision  but  that  it  car- 
ried the  stimulus  that  led  to  the  contractile  response 
of  the  iris  to  light.  In  the  post-mortem  examination 
on  a  child  with  fixed  pupils  he  found  a  lesion  blocking 
the  inflow  from  the  optic  nerves  to  the  thalamus  and 
inferred  that  this  impairment  of  sen.sory  inflow  was 
responsible  for  the  motor  deficit  that  had  been  the 
clinical  sign.  This  was  indeed  the  recognition  of  a 
reflex  arc,  and  the  pupillary  reflex  was  for  many  years 
known  by  his  name. 

As  noted  above,  Willis  had  di.ssected  the  spinal 
accessory  nerve  to  its  junction  with  the  cord  but  he 
believed  it  to  convey  voluntary  control.  Lacking  a 
.scientific  acumen  equal  to  his  skill  as  a  dissector,  and 
influenced  by  Galen,  he  thought  this  nerve  anasto- 
mosed with  the  vagus  (the  "wandering'  nerve). 
Schneider,  on  the  other  hand,  had  no  doubts  as  to 
the  action  of  the  olfactory  nerves  for  it  was  his  work 
on  the  nasal  mucosa  and  olfactory  processes  that  led 
to  his  identification  of  them.  Willis  also  was  aware  of 
their  function  for  he  called  them  the  'smelling'  nerves. 
He  noted  that  within  the  skull  they  had  'mammillary 
processes'  and  said,  "As  to  the  Fibres  and  Filaments 
or  little  strings  stretching  out  from  the  more  soft 
nerves  through  the  holes  of  the  Sieve-like  Bone  into 
the  caverns  of  the  Nose,  these  are  found  in  all  Crea- 
tures who  have  the  mammillary  Processes:  so  it  is 
not  to  be  doubted,  but  that  these  Processes,  with  this 
appendix  and  its  medullary  origine  is  the  Organ  of 
Smell."-'  Willis  called  in  his  knowledge  of  compara- 

"  The  quotations  arc  from  Pordagos  translation  (1683)  of 
Willis,  T.  Cerebri  anatome:  cui  acces\it  nervorum  descnpho  el  usus. 
London:  Flesher,  1664. 


five  anatomy  and  noted  that  "the  filaments  or  little 
strings"  of  the  organ  of  smell  were  "more  remarkable 
in  hunting  Hounds  than  in  any  other  Animal  whatso- 
ever. 

The  ner\es  that  had  ijoth  sensory  and  motor 
branches  proved  the  most  difficult.  Magendie  (238) 
at  first  thought  the  fifth  nerve  was  sensory  and  nutrient 
to  the  face,  and  the  seventh  nerve  entirely  motor, 
since  cutting  it  caused  facial  paralysis  without  reliev- 
ing neuralgia.  In  1820  Charles  Bell  (147),  dissecting 
the  nerves  of  the  face,  noticed  that  the  fibers  of  the 
seventh  nerve  went  to  muscle  whereas  those  of  the 
fifth  entered  the  skin.  He  suspected  they  .served  diff'er- 
ent  functions,  and  being  himself  an  anatomist  rather 
than  an  experimentalist,  asked  his  brother-in-law, 
John  Shaw,  to  make  a  study  of  the  effect  of  sections 
of  these  nerves.  Using  an  unusual  experimental 
animal,  the  donkey,  Shaw  was  able  to  demonstrate 
paralysis  in  the  one  case,  loss  of  reaction  to  touch  in 
the  other;  neither  he  nor  Bell  whose  fine  drawings 
illustrate  his  findings  recognized  the  mixed  nature  of 
these  nerves.  After  this  beginning  several  workers 
added  their  contributions  to  the  further  clarification 
of  the  cranial  nerves,  prominent  among  these  being 
Mayo  (239)  (who  taught  the  course  in  anatomy  and 
physiology  at  King's  College,  London). 

It  was  only  in  the  eighteenth  century  that  doubt 
was  first  thrown  on  the  assumption  that  the  sympa- 
thetic trunk  (or  'intercostal'  nerve,  as  it  was  then 
called)  was  an  appendage  of  the  brain.  This  grew 
from  the  transection  experiments  of  Pourfour  du 
Petit  (240)  and  his  oijsers'ations  on  contraction  of  the 
pupil.  For  centuries  anatomists  had  shown  this  nerve 
as  stemming  from  the  brain.  V'esalius  (7),  in  his 
drawings  of  the  human  nervous  system,  put  it  in  one 
trunk  with  the  vagus.  (In  the  dog,  though  not  in  man, 
the  two  nerves  lie  in  the  same  sheath  in  the  neck 
region.)  Eustachius  (241)  separated  the  two,  but  like 
many  after  him,  including  Willis,  he  depicted  an 
intracranial  origin.  These  drawings  of  the  anatomists 
must  have  been  designed  to  be  consistent  with  Galen- 


■238.   Magendie,  F.  J.  physiol.  exper.  et  path.  4:  176,  302,  1824. 

239.  Mayo,  H.  Anatomical  and  Physiological  Commentaries. 
London:  Underwood,  vol.   I,    1822;  vol.   II,    1823. 

240.  Pourfour  du  Petit,  Franqois  (1664-1741).  Memoire 
dans  lequel  il  est  demonstre  que  les  nerfs  intercostaux 
fournissent  des  rameaux  que  portent  des  esprits  dans  les 
ncrfs.  Hnt.   Acad.  ray.  Sc.  Paris  i,  1727. 

241.  Eustachius,  Bartolommeo  (1520-1574).  Tabulae  anato- 
micae  (posthumous).  Rome :  Gonzaga,  1 7 1 4. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


43 


ist  doctrine,  rather  than  with  observation  I'rom  dis- 
section, du  Petit's  experiments  came  very  close  to 
uncovering  the  action  of  vasomotor  nerves,  the  suij- 
ject  that  was  to  receive  so  much  investigation  in  later 
years  from  Claude  Bernard  (242,  243),  from  Clarl 
Ludwig  (244)  and  from  Pavlov's  other  teacher,  Cyon 
(245).  Bernard's  experiments  were  mostly  on  skin 
temperature  changes  due  to  vasomotor  action,  al- 
though at  no  time  would  he  relinquish  entirely  an 
explanation  on  a  metabolic  basis.  Ludwig  had  found 
the  secretory  action  of  the  lingual  nerve  but  he  did 
not  separate  it  from  the  chorda  tympani  as  Bernard 
did  later. 

A  marked  advance  in  understanding  the  physiol- 
ogy, not  only  of  the  cranial  nerves  but  of  the  brain 
itself,  came  when  techniques  were  developed  for 
ablating  and  stimulating  parts  of  the  central  nervous 
system  without  the  animal  succumbing  to  the  pro- 
cedures. The  surgery  in  the  early  attempts  was  fre- 
quently so  drastic  that  results  were  rarely  specific. 
For  example,  the  experimental  results  of  Willis  that 
confirmed  his  belief  in  the  cerebelliun  as  a  vital 
center  were  probably  due  to  his  animal's  having  suc- 
cumbed to  injuries  near  the  fourth  ventricle.  Other 
early  experimenters  such  as  Duverney  (246)  with  his 
pigeons,  Chirac  (247)  and  Perrault  (248)  with  their 
dogs  had  to  be  satisfied  with  very  brief  durations  of 
survival. 

At  the  opening  of  the  nineteenth  century  interest  in 
localization  of  cerebral  function  had  been  widely 
stirred  by  the  lectures  of  Franz  Gall  (249)  in  Vienna. 
Unfortunately  Gall's  reputation  as  a  phrenologist  has 

242.  Bernard,  Claude  (18 13-1878).  InHucnce  du  Kiand 
sympathique  sur  la  sensibilite  et  sur  la  calorification. 
Cnrnpt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  3;  163,  1851. 

243.  Bernard,  C.  De  I'influence  de  deux  oidies  dc  ncifs  qui 
determine  les  variations  de  couleur  du  sang  veineux  dans 
les  organes  glandulaircs.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,    sc,   Paris  47: 

^4.5.  393.  1858. 

244.  Ludwig,  Carl  Friedrich  VVilhelm  (1816-1895).  Mill, 
naturjorsch.  Gessellsch.  ^urich  50,  1851. 

245.  Cyon,  Ilya  (1842-1912)  and  C.  F.  \V.  Ludwig.  Die 
Reflexe  eincs  der  sensiblen  Nerven  des  Hcizcns  auf  die 
motorischen  der  Blutgefasse.  Arb.  Physiol.  Insl.,  Leipzig  1  : 
128,  1867. 

246.  Duverney,  Joseph  Guichard  (1648-1730).  Phil.  Trans. 
Roy.  Soc.  19;  226,  1697  (reported  by  Preston). 

247.  Chirac,  Pierre  (1650-1732).  Du  niolu  cordis  analylica. 
Montpellier,  1698. 

248.  Perrault,  Claude  (1613-1688).  Mernoires  pour  servir  a 
I'histoire  des  animaux.  Paris:  Acad.  d.  Sci.,  1671-1676. 

249.  Gall,  Franz  Joseph  (1758-1828)  and  Johann  Caspar 
Spurzheim  (1776-1832).  Recherches  sur  le  systeme  ner- 
veux  en  general,  et  sur  celui  du  cerveau  en  particulier. 
Mem.  Inst.  Paris  1808. 


fc: 


fig.  28.  Above:  Gall  and  Spurzheim's  map  of  a  skull  with 
certain  areas  marked  for  correspondence  with  different  mental 
acu  Ities.  Below,  for  comparison :  Gall's  skull  on  the  left,  that  of 
Spurzheim  on  the  right.  Although  Gall's  own  ideas  were  chan- 
neled into  phrenology,  they  were  influential  in  directing  interest 
to  the  study  of  cerebral  localization.  (The  skull  of  Gall  is  in  the 
Musee  de  I'Homme  in  Paris  and  is  reproduced  here  by  the 
kindness  of  Dr.  Ardvege;  that  of  Spurzheim  is  in  the  Warren 
Museum  at  the  Harvard  Medical  School,  and  has  been  photo- 
graphed by  permission  of  Dr.  P.  L  Yakovlev.) 


overshadowed  his  more  important  work  on  the  fiber 
tracts  of  the  white  matter  of  the  brain,  work  which 
clarified  the  pre\iousl\-  contradictory  ideas  as  to  the 
anatomy  of  the  commissures  and  of  the  pvramidal 
decussation.  But,  while  his  contemporaries  were  con- 
cerning themsehcs  with  sites  for  sensory  and  motor 
functions.  Gall  was  propo.sing  localization  of  mental 
faculties  and  he  may  be  regarded  as  a  pioneer  in 
emphasizing  the  importance  of  the  grey  matter  for 
intellectual  processes.  It  was  when,  together  with  his 
pupil,  Spurzheim  (250),  he  proceeded  to  assign 
separate  'organs'  in  the  brain  to  the  different  mental 
faculties  and  to  relate  these  to  bumps  on  the  skull 
that  he  isegan  to  be  challenged.  All  the  same,  in  spite 

250.  Gall,  F.  J.  and  J.  C.  Spurzheim.  Anatomic  et  physiologic 
du  systhne  nerveux  en  general  et  du  cerveau  en  particulier,  avec 
des  observations  intellectuelles  et  morales  de  r hotnme  et  des  ani- 
maux, par  la  configuration  de  leur  teles.  Paris:  Schoell  1810— 
1819  (vols.  I  &  II  by  Gall  &  Spurzheim;  vols.  Ill  &  IV 
by  Gall). 


44 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  '-^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  29.  Two  investigators  of  the  cerebellum,  Pierre  Fiourens 
(1794- 1 867)  and  Luigi  Luciani  (i  840-1 921). 


of  its  bizarre  concepts,  phrenology  had  a  surprisingly 
wide  acceptance  for  a  considerable  period  even 
among  the  medical  profession.  It  was  to  the  psychol- 
ogists (although  that  term  was  not  yet  in  use)  that 
phrenology  particularly  appealed,  for  it  was  the  first 
major  consideration  of  mental  characteristics  as 
attributes  of  brain  function. 

One  of  the  more  prominent  men  to  attack  Gall's 
doctrines  was  Fiourens  who  made  a  sweeping  rejec- 
tion of  all  such  ideas,  denying  the  brain  any  discretely 
localized  action.  But  Fiourens'  monograph  (251) 
appeared  some  years  after  the  deaths  of  Gall  and 
Spurzheim  both  of  whom  had  built  up  comfortable 
careers  out  of  their  speciality.  Fiourens  recognized 
three  major  functional  regions  of  the  brain  (the 
cerebral  hemispheres,  the  medulla  and  the  cere- 
bellum), but  within  these  entities  he  envisaged  their 
action  as  global  and  their  roles  as  being  sensory,  vital 
and  motor,  respectively.  Concerning  the  cerebral 
hemispheres  he  said  that  animals  that  survive  their 
removal  "lose  perception,  judgment,  memory  and 
will  .  .  .  therefore  the  cerebral  hemispheres  are  the 
sole  site  of  perception  and  all  intellectual  abilities" 
(252).  He  did  not  hesitate  to  infer  subjective  qualities 
and  faculties.  In  one  of  the  more  renowned  of  his 
experiments  (253)  he  had  kept  a  pigeon  alive  after 
removal   of  its  cerebral   hemispheres.   The  bird  was 

■251.  Flourens,  Pierre  (1794-1867).  Examai  de  Phremlogie. 
Paris,  1842;  English  translation  by  D.  de  L.  Meigs. 
Phrenology  Examined.  Philadelphia,  1846. 

252.  Flourens,  P.  Recherches  experimentales  sur  les  proprietes  et  les 
fonctions  du  systeme  nerveux  dans  les  animaux  verlebres.  Paris: 
Crevot,  1824. 

253.  Flourens,  P.  Arcli.  gen,  de  med.  2:  321,  1823. 


'blind'  and  'deaf  and  appeared  to  be  asleep  although 
it  stirred  when  poked.  Flourens  went  so  far  as  to  say 
that  the  bird  lost  its  volition  and  "even  the  faculty  of 
dreaming."  He  noted  that  it  retained  the  sense  of 
equilibrium  and  that  its  pupils  still  reacted  to  light. 
Others  repeating  Flourens'  experiments  were  uncon- 
vinced, for  their  decerebrate  pigeons  could  be  starded 
by  a  loud  noise  and  could  avoid  obstacles. 

Since  sudden  death  followed  section  of  the  medulla, 
Flourens  concluded  that  here  lay  the  essential  mecha- 
nism for  respiration  and  the  maintenance  of  life.  In 
this  conclusion  he  had  of  course  been  anticipated  by 
Legallois.  Much  of  Flourens'  fame  as  an  experimental- 
ist derived  from  his  observation  that  extirpation  of  the 
cerebellum  (in  birds  and  mammals)  caused  loss  of 
coordinated  movement.  Flourens,  who.se  interest  lay 
so  deeply  in  the  elucidation  of  the  control  of  voluntary 
movement,  was  himself  to  suffer  paralysis  for  a  long 
period  before  his  death. 

In  the  1820's  when  Fluorens  was  pursuing  these 
experiments,  many  workers  were  'mutilating'  ani- 
mals (to  use  Gall's  phrase)  (254),  and  some  jockeying 
for  priority  was  inevitai)le.  Most  of  Flourens'  observa- 
tions, particularly  those  on  the  cerebellum,  had  been 
anticipated  by  Rolando  at  Sassari,  whose  treatise 
(255)  of  1809  (written  in  the  Italian  language  and 
printed  and  illustrated  ijy  himself)  was  therefore  re- 
published in  French  in  an  abbreviated  form  in  1824 
C256). 

Rolando  did  not  succeed  in  keeping  his  animals 
alive.  Even  his  tortoises  died  after  removal  of  their 
brains,  although  Fontana  who  had  been  successful 
with  these  animals  showed  him  his  own  technique. 
Many  of  Rolando's  conclusions  (257)  were  therefore 
incorrect  since  he  mistook  surgical  shock  for  paralysis. 
Less  ruthless  extirpations,  of  the  hemispheres  only,  he 
found  to  be  compatible  with  life.  Rolando  believed 
the  cerebellum  to  be  a  kind  of  'voltaic  pile'  and  the 
source  of  all  movement.  Flourens  thought  it  merely 
the  regulator.  Magendie  (258)  disagreed,  holding 
cerebellar  function  to  be  maintenance  of  equilibrium. 

254.  Gall,  F.  J.  Stir  les  fonctions  du  cerveau  et  sur  eelles  de  chacune 
de  ses  parties.  Paris,   1822-1825.  6  vol. 

255.  Rolando,  Luigi  (1773-1831).  Saggio  sopra  la  vera  strutlura 
del  cervello  delV  uorno  de  degi  animali  e  sopra  le  funzioni  del 
ststerna  nervoso.  Sassari,   1 809. 

256.  Rolando,  L.  Experiences  sur  les  fonctions  du  systeme 
nerveu.x.  J.  physiol.  exper.  et  path.  3:  95,  1823. 

257.  Rolando,  L.  Osservazioni  sul  cervelletto.  Mem.  reale 
aecad.  sc.  Turin  29:  163,  1825. 

258.  Magendie,  F.  Precis  elhnenlaire  de  Physiologic.  Paris,  1825; 
English  translation  by  E.  Mulligan.  Edinburgh:  C^arfrae, 
1826. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY  45 


FIG.  30.  Lejt:  Magendie's  technique  for  sectioning  the  fifth  nerve  in  the  living  rabbit.  The  dissec- 
tion is  to  demonstrate  the  insertion  of  his  instrument.  On  the  rabbit's  right,  the  probe  is  seen  entering 
the  base  of  the  siiull  and  reaching  the  trunk  of  the  fifth  nerve  at  H.  On  the  animal's  left,  the  end 
of  the  instrument  is  seen  at  E  and  the  sectioned  nerve  at  G.  (From :  Bernard,  C.  Leqons  sur  la  Pkysiologie 
el  la  Pathologif  du  Sysleme  .^eri'eux.  Paris:  Bailliere,  1858.  Right:  pigeon  deprived  of  its  cerebral  hemi- 
spheres in  position  described  by  Flourens.  (From:  Luciani,  L.  Human  Physiology,  English  ed.  Lon- 
don: Macmillan,  1915.) 


He  reached  this  conclusion  from  studying  the  dis- 
turbance of  gait  in  a  duck-*  from  which  he  had  re- 
removed  the  cerebellum  unilaterally.  He  followed 
these  experiments  with  bilateral  destructions  and 
noticed  forced  movements.  The  great  contribution 
towards  our  modern  knowledge  of  cerebellar  mecha- 
nisms came  from  Luciani  of  Florence  whose  book  // 
Cervellelto  (259)  is  a  classic,  as  is  also  his  te.xtbook  of 
physiology  (260). 

Magendie  in  the  obsersations  he  made  on  decere- 
brate animals  (261)  anticipated  Sherrington  by  an 
accurate  and  detailed  description  of  decerebrate 
rigidity  in  rabbits.  This  was  in  the  days  before  the 
discovery  of  anesthesia  and  Magendie  was  severely 

.■59.  Luciani,  Luigi  (1840-1921).  //  Cervelletto.  Florence,  1891. 
260.   Luciani,  L.  Human  Physiology.  English  translation  by  F.  A. 

Welby.  London:  Macmillan,   1915. 
■261.   Magkndie,  F.  Sur  le  siege  du  mouvement  et  du  sentiment 

dans  la  moelle  epiniere.  J.  phvsiol.  cxper.  ct  path.  3;   153, 

1823. 


criticized  for  his  practice  of  vivisection.  But  extirpa- 
tion experiments  on  animals  could  give  no  clue  to  the 
cortical  representation  of  speech.  This  had  to  come 
from  clinical  observation  with  studies  at  autopsy. 
Gall  had  placed  language  in  the  anterior  lobes  and  the 
first  clinical  reports  seemed  to  confirm  this.  In  fact, 
the  great  surge  of  work  aiming  to  establish  localized 
centers  in  the  human  brain  began  with  the  speech 
center.  In  his  studies  of  encephalitis  Bouillaud  (262}, 
a  pupil  of  Magendie  and  later  Professor  of  Medicine, 
had  reasoned  that  the  anterior  lobes  of  the  brain  were 
necessary  for  speech  and  went  on  to  ob.serve  that 
other  focal  lesions  of  the  brain  caused  localized  im- 

262.  Bouillaud,  Jean  Baptiste  (i  796-1 881).  Traite  clinique  et 
physiologique  de  V encephalitt'  ou  inflammation  du  cerveau.  Paris : 
Bailliere,  1825. 


^'  Sherrington    in    quoting    this    experiment    mistranslated 
Magendie's  word  'canard'  as  'water-dog.' 


46  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


'i-yanuC 


Strasssui 


FIG.  31.  Goltz  and  one  of  his  decorticate  dogs.  (Studio  portraits  of  man  and  dog  are  reproduced 
here  by  the  tcind  permission  of  Dr.  Paul  Dell.) 


pairment  of  mu.scular  movement.  The  cause  of  cere- 
bral localization  was  taken  up  by  his  son-in-law, 
Auburtin  (263),  who  predicted  that  a  lesion  would 
be  found  in  the  anterior  lobes  of  an  aphasic  patient 
who  was  at  that  time  in  the  hospital  of  Bicetre  under 
the  surgeon  Pierre  Broca.  Autopsy  confirmed  Aubur- 
tin's  prediction,  pinpointing  the  lesion  in  the  left 
anterior  lobe.  The  next  aphasic  patient  on  Broca's 
service  was  found  at  autopsy  to  have  an  even  more 
discrete  lesion — in  what  is  known  to  this  day  as 
Broca's  area  ('-64)-  The  name  of  Auburtin  has  been 
forgotten,  as  has  Broca's  term  'aphemia'  for  aphasia. 

Broca's  speech  area  (the  left  third  frontal  convolu- 
tion) which  he  thought  to  be  concerned  with  articula- 
tion was  to  be  challenged  by  Pierre  Marie  (265)  in 
the  twentieth  century,  but  the  new  concept  of  cerebral 
localization  de\eloped  like  a  wave  in  the  later  1800's 
• — a  wave  that   is  only   now   beginning  partially   to 

263.  AuBERTiN,  Ernst  (18-25-  )■  Considerations  sur  les 
localisations  cerebrales,  et  en  particulier  sur  le  siege  de  la 
faculte  du  langage  articule.  Ga:^.  hehd.  med.  et  chir.  10:  318, 
348,  397.  455.  1863. 

264.  Broca,  Pierre  Paul  (1824-1880).  Perte  dc  parole, 
ramoUissement  chronique  et  destruction  du  lobe  anterieur 
gauche  du  cerveau.  Bull.  soc.  anthropol.  Paris  2:  235,  1861. 

265.  Marie,  Pierre  (1853- 1940).  Revision  de  la  question  de 
I'aphasie;  la  troisieme  circonvolution  frontale  gauche  ne 
joue  aucun  role  special  dans  la  fonction  du  langage.  Sem. 
med.  Paris  26:  241,  1906. 


recede.  For  the  physiologists  the  impressive  experi- 
ments were  those  of  Goltz  of  Strasbourg  who,  after 
starting  with  frogs  (266},  mastered  the  technique  of 
keeping  warm-blooded  animals  ali\e  for  prolonged 
periods  after  drastic  extirpations  of  large  portions  of 
their  brains  (267).  Three  of  his  dogs  became  famous. 
The  first  two  survived  57  and  92  days  respectively, 
the  third  being  purposely  sacrificed  at  18  months. 
Goltz  exhibited  them  at  international  congresses, 
killed  one  of  them  before  an  audience  and  gave  their 
brains  to  Langley  in  Foster's  laboratory  to  dissect 
(268,  269).  Sherrington's  participation  in  the  necropsy 
of  one  of  these  dogs  was  the  subject  of  his  first  pub- 
lished paper  (in  1884)  (270).  All  who  witnessed  the 
remarkable    degree    of   retention    of   sensibility    and 

266.  Goltz,  Friedrich  Leopold  (1834-1902).  Beilrcige  z"r 
Lehre  den  Funktionen  der  Nervenz.enlren  des  Frosches.  Berlin : 
Hirschwald,  i86g. 

267.  Goltz,  F.  L.  Der  Hund  ohne  Grosshirn.  .irch.  ges.  Physiol. 
51:  570,  1892. 

268.  Langlev,  J.  N.  Report  on  the  parts  destroyed  on  the 
right  side  of  the  brain  of  the  dog  operated  on  by  Professor 
Goltz.  J.  Physiol.  4:  286,  1883. 

269.  L.ANGLEV,  J.  N.  AND  A.  S.  Grunbaum.  On  the  degenera- 
tion resulting  from  removal  of  the  cerebral  cortex  and 
corpora  striata  in  the  dog.  J.  Physiol.  1 1 :  606,  1890. 

270.  Langlev,  J.  N.  and  0.  S.  Sherrington.  Secondary  de- 
generation of  nerve  tracts  following  removal  of  the  cortex 
of  the  cerebrum  in  the  dog.  J.  Physiol.  5:  49,  1884. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLDGV 


47 


mobility  by  these  animals  and  who  later  studied  the 
necropsy  findings  from  the  Cambridge  laboratory 
were  astounded,  and  there  can  be  no  doubt  that  these 
experiments  gave  a  great  impetus  to  neurosurgical 
procedures  in  animals  and  in  man. 

The  physiology  of  the  brain  was  now  beginning  to 
unfold  and  to  reveal  itself  in  dynamic  terms  after 
centuries  of  static  representation  in  the  two-dimen- 
sional pages  of  the  anatomy  books.  To  clinical  obser- 
vation of  impairment  by  disease  states,  three  experi- 
mental techniques  were  added :  regional  ablation, 
stimulation  (both  mechanical  and  electrical)  and 
eventualh'  the  recording  of  the  brain's  own  electricity. 

Mechanical  and  chemical  irritation  of  the  cortical 
surface  had  suggested  itself  to  many  in\estigators  down 
the  years,  some  of  the  attempts  reaching  the  extremes 
of  the  bizarre  (see,  for  example,  fig  32).  Cabanis 
(271),  the  celebrated  physician  and  ideologue,  had 
provoked  convulsive  movements  in  muscle  groups 
that  .seemed  to  vary  with  the  region  irritated.  Earlier, 
Haller  (272),  searching  for  irritability,  had  pricked 
the  brain  and  applied  irritating  fluids  and  concluded 
that  the  grey  matter  was  insensitive  to  stimulation 
and  that  the  white  matter  was  the  seat  of  sensation 
and  the  source  of  movement. 

The  Italian  physiologists  had  been  more  successful. 
The  Abbe  Fontana  (273)  and  Caldani  (274)  (Gal- 
vani's  predecessor  in  the  chair  of  anatomy  at  Bologna) 
had  convulsed  their  frogs  by  electrical  stimulation 
inside  their  brains.  Rolando  (255),  following  their 
lead,  extended  his  experiments  to  pigs,  goats,  sheep, 
dogs  and  also  to  birds.  The  influential  Magendie 
however  had  failed  and  had  proclaimed  the  cortex 
electrically  inexcitable;  an  opinion  in  which  he  was 
backed  by  Flourens  (252).  In  these  days  before  the 
neuron  had  been  recognized  as  the  unit  of  the  nervous 
system,  before  the  pyramidal  fibers  were  known  to  be 
processes  of  cortical  cells,  there  was  no  a  priori  reason 
to  expect  electrical  stimulation  of  the  cortical  surface 
to  have  a  peripheral  effect,  but  soon  an  incontro- 
\ertible  proof  was  to  be  given. 


FIG.  32.  One  of  the  bizarre  experiments  of  .Mdini  on  two 
freshly-decapitated  criminals.  In  the  center  is  a  voltaic  pile,  the 
circuit  through  the  heads  being  completed  by  conducting 
arcs.  .Mdini,  Galvani's  impetuous  nephew,  lacked  the  sagacity 
and  scientific  acumen  of  his  famous  uncle.  (From  Aldini,  G. 
Essai  Theorique  el  Experimental  sur  le  Galvanisme.  Paris;  Fournier, 
1804.  2  vol.) 


FIG.  33.  Two  pioneers  in  attempts  to  stimulate  the  brain: 
the  Abbe  Fontana,  physician  to  the  Archduke  of  Tuscany  and 
professor  of  physics  in  the  University  of  Pisa;  and  Caldani, 
Galvani's  predecessor  in  the  chair  of  anatomy  at  Bologna. 
(The  portrait  of  Fontana  is  reproduced  by  courtesy  of  Dr.  G. 
Pupilli.) 


271.  Cabanis,  Pierre  J.-G.  Rapports  du  physique  et  du  moral  de 
I'homme.  Paris:  Bibliotheque  Choisie,   1830. 

272.  ZiNN,  JoHANN  Gottfried  (i  727-1 759)  and  A.  Haller. 
Memoir es  sur  les  parties  sensibles  et  trrilables  du  corps  animal. 
Lausanne:  D'Arnay,  1760. 

273.  Fontana,  Felice  (1720-1805).  Acead.  Sc.  1st.  Bologna, 
>757- 

274.  Caldani,  Leopoldo  (1725-18 13).  Institutiones  phystologicae 
et  pathologicae.  Leyden:  Luchtmans,  1784. 


The  pioneers  were  Fritsch  &  Hitzig  (275)  (two 
young  privatdocents  in  Berlin)  with  their  now  famous 
experiments  in  which  they  u.sed  a  galvanic  current 
and  from  which  evolved  the  idea  of  a  'motor  cortex.* 

275.  Fritsch,  Gust.w  Theodor  (1838-1891)  and  Eduard 
Hitzig  (1838-1907).  Uber  die  elektrische  Erregbarkeit 
des  Grosshirns.  .Irch.  Anal.  Physiol,  miss.  Med.  Leipzig  37: 
300,    1870. 


48 


HANDBOOK    OF    Pm-SIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


fXi^*^:\m^.l 


FIG.  34.  Gustav  Fritsch  and  Edouard  Hitzig.  (Photographs 
reproduced  by  kind  permission  of  Dr.  A.  E.  Walker,  for  whom 
Professor  Stender  of  Berlin  obtained  the  picture  of  Hitzig.) 


Ferrier  (276-278),  a  few  years  later,  in  a  long  series 
of  experiments  using  faradic  stimulation  in  monkeys 
was  able  to  bring  out  not  merely  muscle  twitches  of  an 
indeterminate  kind  but  also  grosser  movements.  Of 
course,  as  we  now  know,  these  are  imprecise  and  even 
athetoid  in  comparison  with  movements  made  by  the 
animal  naturally.  Benefitting  from  the  parallel  devel- 
opment of  electrical  techniques,  Victor  Horsley,  in  a 
series  of  papers  with  Beevor  (279,  280)  in  the  next 
decade,  described  more  closely  the  motor  areas  in  the 
monkey  cortex.  From  these  experiments  there  emerged 
the  designation  of  the  precentral  gyrus  as  predomi- 
nantly motor  in  function  and  the  postcentral  as  sen- 
sory.   Between    the    two,    Beevor   &    Horsley    (281, 

^76.  Ferrier,  David  (1843-1928).  The  localization  of  function 
in  the  brain.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  22:  229,  1873-4. 

277.  Ferrier,  D.  Experiments  on  the  brain  of  monkeys.  P/ut. 
Trans.  165:433,  1876. 

278.  Ferrier,  D.  The  Function  of  Ihf  Brain.  London  Smith 
Elder,  1876. 

279.  Beevor,  C.  E.  and  V.  Horsley.  A  minute  analysis  (ex- 
perimental) of  the  various  movements  produced  by  stim- 
ulating in  the  monkey  different  regions  of  the  cortical 
centre  for  the  upper  limb  as  defined  by  Professor  Ferrier. 
Phil.  Trans.  178:  153,  1887. 

280.  Beevor,  C.  E.  and  V.  Horsley.  A  further  minute  analy- 
sis by  electrical  stimulation  of  the  so-called  motor  region 
(facial  area)  of  the  cortex  cerebri  in  the  monkey.  Phil. 
Trans.  185:  39,  1894. 

281.  Beevor,  C.  E.  and  V.  Horsley.  An  experimental  in- 
vestigation into  the  arrangement  of  the  excitable  fibres  of 


282)  recognized  an  area  which  they  called  'the  zone 
of  confusion.'  An  important  point  that  emerged  from 
their  use  of  this  technique  was  that  in  addition  to  areas 
of  maximal  representation  of  a  given  movement,  the 
cortex  also  has  marginal  zones  that  are  less  specific. 
In  other  words,  they  found  no  sharp  demarcation 
lines. 

With  Schaefer  (283),  Horsley  went  on  to  further 
studies  of  both  motor  and  sensory  function,  using 
ablation  as  well  as  electrical  excitation.  The  basic 
interest  was  of  course  in  the  application  of  these  find- 
ings to  man,  especially  in  the  light  of  the  observations 
of  Hughlings  Jackson  on  the  march  of  symptoms 
during  the  epileptic  fit  (284).  Species  differences 
came  markedly  to  light  when  Beevor  &  Horsley 
compared  their  findings  on  the  bonnet  monkey  with 
those  in  the  orangutan.  The  first  pioneers  to  attempt 
electrical  stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  man  (through 
holes  in  the  skull)  were  Bartholow  in  America  in 
1874  (285)  and  Sciamanna  8  years  later  in  Italy  (286). 
These  were  followed  by  Keen  (287),  in  his  youth  an 
army  surgeon  in  the  American  Civil  War  and  later 
professor  of  surgery  at  Jefferson  Medical  College.  In 
1888,  in  a  patient  whose  seizures  began  in  the  hand, 
he  removed  the  area  the  stimulation  of  which  caused 
movements  of  the  wrist.  He  used  a  'faradic  battery,' 
and  with  it  found  areas  for  hand,  elbow,  shoulder 
and  face  movements.  When  respiration  and  circula- 
tion became  poor,  he  revived  the  patient  with  brandy 
injected  into  the  forearm.  In  the  same  year  several 
other  workers  applied  a  similar  technique  in  man  but 


the  internal  capsule  of  the  bonnet  monkey.  Pliil.    Trans. 
181 :  49,  1890. 

282.  Beevor,  C.  E.  and  V.  Horsley.  A  record  of  the  results 
obtained  by  electrical  excitation  of  the  so-called  motor 
cortex  and  internal  capsule  in  the  orang-utang.  Phil. 
Trans.  i8i :  129,  i8go. 

283.  Horsley,  V.  and  Edward  Albert  Schaefer  (1850- 
1935).  A  record  of  experiments  upon  the  functions  of  the 
cerebral  cortex.  Phil.  Trans.  179:  i,  1888. 

284.  Jackson,  John  Hughlings  (1835-191  i).  Unilateral 
epileptiform  seizures,  attended  by  temporary  defect  of 
sight.  Med.   Times  Gaz.   I  :  588,   1863. 

285.  Bartholow,  Roberts  (1831-1904).  Experimental  in- 
vestigations into  the  functions  of  the  human  brain.  .Im. 
J.  M.&.  67:305,  1874. 

286.  Sciamanna,  E.  Gli  avversari  delle  localizzazioni  cercbrali. 
Arch,  psichiat.  Turin  3:  209,  1882. 

287.  Keen,  William  Williams  (1837-1932).  Three  successful 
cases  of  cerebral  surgery  including  (i)  The  removal  of  a 
large  intracranial  fibroma;  (2)  Exsection  of  damaged 
brain  tissue;  and  (3)  Exsection  of  the  cerebral  centre  for 
the  left  hand;  with  remarks  on  the  general  technique  of 
such  operations.  Am.  J.  M.  Sc.  96;  329,  452,  1888. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


49 


^P^^l 

P^^^ 

v.^,           \_rrg<^ 

HHHHHH^^HHIi^^^Hiiii^wiKd^^-.-je^^  ^ 

FIG.  35.  Victor  Horsley  and  one  of  his  experiments  on  the  locaUzation  of  the  motor  cortex.  (The 
latter  illustration  from  Trans.  Congr.  Am.  Physic.  Surg,  i  :  340,  1888.) 


systematic  exploration  had  to  wait  for  Gushing, 
Foerster  and  Penfield  in  the  modern  age  of  neuro- 
surgery, and  for  the  development  of  clinical  neuro- 
physiological  investigation. 

In  the  light  of  clinical  oljser\ation  and  the  results  of 
electrical  stimulation,  the  concept  that  the  cortical 
grey  matter  acted  as  a  whole  and  that  motor  function 
had  no  representation  above  the  basal  ganglia  began 
to  crumble.  At  this  same  period,  the  birth  of  a  new 
technique  brought  yet  another  method  of  approach 
for  the  investigator.  This  was  the  recording  of  brain 
potentials  evoked  by  sen.sorv  stimulation  and  the 
discovery  of  the  Ijrain's  own  electrical  activity,  the 
dawn  of  electroencephalography. 

In  1875  Richard  Caton  (288),  at  the  Royal  Infir- 
mary School  of  Medicine  in  Liverpool,  while  searching 
for  the  cerebral  counterpart  of  du  Bois-Reymond's 
action  potential  in  nerve,  not  only  found  it,  but 
noticed  that  when  both  of  his  electrodes  lay  on  the 
cortical  surface  there  was  a  continuous  waxing  and 
waning  of  potential.  This  oscillation  of  the  base  line 
was  present  in  the  unstimulated  animal  and  Caton 
proved  it  to  be  unrelated  to  respiratory  or  cardiac 
rhythms.  He  also  proved  these  fluctuations  to  be 
biological  in  origin  ijy  showing  them  to  be  vulnerable 

288.   Caton,   Rich.^rd  (1842-1926).   The  electric  currents  of 
the  brain.  Brit.  M.  J.  2:  278,  1875. 


to  anoxia  and  to  anesthesia  and  to  be  abolished  by 
death  of  the  animal.  In  his  first  work  Caton's  experi- 
mental animal  was  the  rabbit  and  his  detecting 
instrument  was  a  Thomson's  galvanometer.  This 
was  in  the  days  before  photographic  recording  of 
laboratory  observations  and  Caton's  first  publication 
of  his  findings  took  the  form  of  a  demonstration  before 
the  British  Medical  Association  (289).  Superimposed 
on  these  o.scillations  Caton  found  potential  swings 
related  to  sensory  stimulation  and  realized  immedi- 
ately the  meaning  of  this  for  cerebral  localization 
studies.  Caton  went  on  to  use  monkeys  and  gave  fur- 
ther reports  of  his  results  in  1877  and  in  1887  (290). 
the  latter  at  the  International  Medical  Congress  held 
that  year  in  Washington,  D.  C. 

Strangely  enough,  in  spite  of  the  prominent  groups 
before  whom  Caton  gas'e  his  demonstrations  and  the 
popular  medical  journal  in  which  he  reported  them, 
his  work  received  little  attention  at  the  time,  even 
among  English-speaking  physiologists.  Meanwhile  in 
Poland,  a  young  a.ssistant  in  the  physiology  depart- 
ment   of  the  University  of  Jagiellonski    in   Krakow, 

289.  Caton,  R.  Interim  report  on  investigation  of  the  electric 
currents  of  the  brain.  Brit.  M.J.  i  :  Suppl.  62,  1877. 

290.  Caton,  R.  Researches  on  electrical  phenomena  of  cere- 
bral grey  matter.  Tr.  Ninth  Internat.  Med.  Congr.  3 :  246, 
1B87. 


50 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  36.  Richard  Caton,  shown  in  his  thirties  at  the  period 
when  he  was  working  in  electrophysiology.  (From  a  photo- 
graph in  possession  of  the  writer,  being  the  generous  gift  of 
Miss  Anne  Caton.) 


Adolf  Beck,  not  knowing  of  Caton's  work  15  years 
earlier,  was  searching  initially  for  the  same  phenom- 
enon, namely  for  electrical  signs  in  the  brain  of  im- 
pulses reaching  it  froin  the  periphery.  Like  Caton 
before  him  he  succeeded,  but  he  also  found  the  brain 
wave.  His  animals  were  mostly  dogs  and  he  pub- 
lished the  protocols  of  all  his  experiments  in  the 
Polish  language  for  a  doctoral  thesis  (291).  In  order 
to  reach  a  wider  audience  he  sent  a  short  account  to 
the  most  widely  read  journal  in  Germany,  the 
Cenlralblatt  jur  Physiologie  (292).  A  spate  of  claims  for 
priority  for  finding  sensorily  evoked  potentials  fol- 
lowed the  German  publication  of  Beck's  findings — 
the  first  coming  from  Fleischl  von  Marxow,  Profes.sor 
of  Physiology  in  Vienna  (293),   and   the  next  from 

291.  Beck,  .Adolf  ( 1863-1942).  O-z'tacz^nie  lokalizocyi  w  moz^u 
i  rdzt'niu  za  ponwca  zj^^^'i^^^  elektry  czynch  (Thesis).  Krakow; 
Univ.  Jagiellonski,  1890. 

Q92.  Beck,  A.  Die  Bestiinmung  der  Localisation  der  Gehirn 
und  Riichenmarksfunktionin  vermittclst  der  elektrischen 
Erscheinungen.  Centrathl.  Physiol.  4:  473,  1890. 

293.  Fleischl  von  Marxow,  Ernst.  Mittheilung  betrefTend 
die  Physiologie  der  Hirnrinde  (letter  to  the  editor  dated 
Vienna,  Nov.  24,  1890).  Centralbl.  Physiol.  4:  537,  1890. 


Gotch  and  Horsley  (294).  It  is  noticeable  that  it  was 
the  electrical  response  of  the  brain  to  sensory  stimula- 
tion that  drew  the  most  interest,  for  this  was  a  finding 
that  lay  directly  in  the  main  stream  of  current  think- 
ing about  cortical  localization  of  function.  The 
completely  novel  idea  of  a  continuously  fluctuating 
electrical  potential  intrinsic  to  the  'resting'  brain 
was,  at  that  time,  of  interest  only  to  its  two  independ- 
ent discoverers,  Caton  and  Beck. 

The  somewhat  acrimonious  wrangle  over  priority 
was  based  in  Fleischl  von  Marxow's  ca.se  on  work 
done  in  1883.  This  had  not  been  published  but  only 
noted  down  in  a  sealed  letter  which  he  had  deposited 
with  the  University  and  which  he  asked  to  have 
opened  after  reading  Beck's  report  in  1890.  He  was 
solely  concerned  with  response  potentials  and  noted 
"little  or  no  movement  of  the  base  line."  He  was 
clearly  unaware  of  Caton's  reports  and  demonstra- 
tions. Gotch  and  Horsley's  ignorance  of  their  country- 
man's work  is  less  easily  understood.  Caton  was  a 
prominent  figure  at  Liverpool,  the  first  holder  of  the 
Chair  of  Physiology  in  which  Gotch  was  to  follow  him 
(and  later  Sherrington). 

The  dispute  in  the  columns  of  the  Centralblalt  o\er 
priority  for  discovery  of  the  electrical  currents  of  the 
brain  was  finally  stilled  by  a  letter  from  Caton  (295), 
drawing  the  attention  of  the  protagonists  to  his 
publication  of  1 5  years  earlier.  By  the  turn  of  the 
century  the  electrical  activity  of  the  brain  had  reached 
the  textijooks  (296).  Caton's  interests  had  developed 
along  many  lines  and  he  became  prominent  in 
.se\eral  fields  of  medicine  and  scholarship  as  well  as  in 
public  affairs,  becoming  in  turn  President  of  the  Medi- 
cal Institution  and  Lord  Mayor  of  Liverpool.  Beck 
(297),  who  at  the  age  of  32  became  professor  of 
Physiology  at  the  University  of  Lvov,  continued  to 
work  on  the  subject  into  this  century,  publishing  with 
his  old  professor  Cybulski,  and  interest  was  thereby 
aroused  in  Germany  and  in  Russia.  He  met  a  tragic 
death  during  the  German  occupation  of  Poland. 

294.  Gotch,  F.  .and  V.  Horslev.  Uber  den  Gebrauch  der 
Elcktricitat  fiir  die  Localzirung  der  Erregungserscheinun- 
gen  im  Centralnervensystem  (letter  to  the  editor  received 
Jan.  17,  1891).  Cenlralbl.  Physiol.  4:649,  1891. 

295.  Caton,  R.  Die  Strome  des  Centralnervensystems  (letter 
to  the  editor  received  Feb.  22,  1891).  Cenlralbl.  Physiol.  4: 
785,  189!. 

296.  ScHAFER,  E.  .\.  Ti'xlhook  of  Physiology.  Edinburgh:  Young 
&  Pentland,  London:  Morrison  &  Gibb;  1898,  1900.  2  vol. 

297.  Beck,  A.  and  Napoleon  Cvbulskl  VVeitere  L'ntersuchun- 
gen  iiber  die  elektrischen  Erscheinungen  in  der  Hirnrinde 
der  .AflTen  und  Hunde.  Cenlralbl.  Physiol.  6:1,  1892. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


51 


ll..-w,.,,l,    ,,„,,       IX 

IN.'-*  *in/y   u 

.■kiirfir\/.i>M:iii\.      P'ifkul.i    iiiM/;;,nv.n   pra^^.i      .lr<lria 

p|«klr»<U  im  "lH/.;ip/ 

w/r'»k'iw\m    ,r.   'IniL';!   11:1   Mlf»/..ir/.'   tii''t.irvf/.iivMi 

dl» 

[•nwdiii'-j  k"ric/.viiv   *"    liir   17>""i     ( i4tf;rlo!ii'  olm  i-li-kir-Ml  ,in  nmi 

A 

Wvrt.vl.iiir:    i'l.    ',o.    l.t.  ;i.\  3M.    in.    ;.\  ;Mh. 

I 

/■.rtilnztiiono  swialfcin   nia;:n<'Wi'ni  «>ko  lewf   !H; 
•|Mi   (intitiiiu  ilra7in.-nia;   i*.'t.  ;U).  2."»,  in.  -i"J.  l.'t.   lO; 
.irariiirnir   ndii'ip    [irzrdnifj    70; 
|K)   ii«wniii  <ln»7.iiii'nta:    ">iS.  IHI,  ."m,  4H- 

■|  r^/i 

A)   Po  (UuTBWJ   |iaii«ic  |im>^aiii;to  tvlna  eloktro- 

Tu  '^ 

dt,>  0  '►  mm.  kii  lyjowt  (Hk-    17  rw).    Wychylenie 

i'fy 

wyk«7.ywftl.>.    70    ok'.liia     la    j.  .-t   .kklrv-iijcriina; 

wvnDtilu  'trK- : 

-^^^  y 

L>ti."..  IM,"..   IH.-,.    isH.  iT.i.  IT.'i.  IMt.  ir.:,.  l.V>,  Ifil; 

~ 

u 

(K)   ziiilra/nieniii   iwiatJcni .    I>2; 

l><i   uilaiiin   draznun-a;    IV..    Il">.    l.'iti.    I.')*t,    I'i7; 

lirzv   (Int/niriiiii   'iJiii»;:i    pr^'- Inirj :    Ihii; 

. 

fij  1: 

|><i   U'<taiiiii    dra/nii-nia :    1  I't.    1  1" 

FIG.  37.  Beck  and  protocol  from  one  of  the  experiments  in  his  original  thesis  on  the  electrical 
phenomena  of  the  brain  and  spinal  cord,  i8go.  (Obtained  through  the  courtesy  of  Dr.  Andrei  Jus  of 
Pruszkow.) 


Interest  became  widespread  in  1929  with  the  first 
publication  on  brain  potentials  in  man.  In  that  year 
Hans  Berger  (298),  a  psychiatrist  in  a  hospital  in 
Jena,  revealed  to  the  scientific  world  the  results  of 
work  he  had  been  pursuing  in  secretive  seclusion  for 
over  5  years.  He  had  repeated  and  confirmed  the 
findings  of  Caton  (to  whom  he  gave  full  credit)  and 
had  extended  them  to  man.  He  studied  (and  named) 
the  electroencephalogram  in  normal  man,  finding 
the  two  major  rhythms,  alpha  and  beta,  that  Nemin- 
ski  had  found  in  dogs  (299).  He  applied  Caton's 
tests  for  the  biological  origin  of  the  potentials  he  found, 
showing  them  to  be  affected  by  hypoxia  and  by  anes- 
thesia. He  also  found  them  to  be  changed  by  sleep. 

Berger's  outstanding  contribution  was  the  founda- 
tion of  clinical  electroencephalography.  Having 
proved  that  brain  waves  could  be  recorded  in  man 
through  the  unopened  skull,  he  went  on  to  demon- 
strate that  their  characteristics  could  be  used  as  an 
index  of  brain  disease  and  thus  he  opened  up  a  new 
line  of  approach  for  the  physiologist  and  the  clinician 
to  the  study  of  brain  mechanisms.  Berger's  major  dis- 
covery in  the  clinical  field  was  that  the  electroen- 
cephalogram is  abnormal  in  epilepsy.  He  did  not  with 

298.  Berger,  Hans  (i 873-1 941).  Uber  das  Elektrenkephalo- 
gramm  des  Menschen.  Arch.  Psychial.  87:  527,  1929. 

299.  Prawditz-Neminski,  W.  W.  Zur  Kenntnis  der  elektrischen 
und  der  Innervationsvorgange  in  den  functionellen 
Elementen  und  Geweben  des  tierischen  Organismus. 
Elektrocerebrogramm  der  Saugcrtiere.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol. 
209:  362,  1925. 


ii 

FIG.  38.  Hans  Berger,  the  first  to  record  electroencephalo- 
graphic  potentials  from  man,  and  the  founder  of  clinical 
electroencephalography.  Below  is  the  first  published  electro- 
encephalogram of  man.  The  subject  was  Berger's  son,  Klaus. 
His  alpha  rhythm  is  shown  in  the  upper  trace  above  a  10  per 
sec.  sine  wave  from  an  oscillator. 

centainty  record  the  spikes  that  are  now  associated 
with  the  seizure  discharge,  for  with  the  technique  he 
used  there  was  serious  interference  by  muscle  poten- 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tials.  His  instruments  were  a  double-coil  galvano- 
meter and  a  string  galvanometer,  and  in  much  of  his 
initial  work  he  used  only  two  electrodes,  these  being 
large  plates  fixed  one  to  the  forehead  and  one  to  the 
back  of  the  head.  He  thus  missed  the  localizing  poten- 
tialities of  the  EEG,  and  in  addition  gathered  in  all 
the  muscle  potentials  of  the  frontalis  and  trapezius 
muscles.  In  later  experiments  he  changed  to  needle 
electrodes  pushed  into  the  skin.  In  his  early  experi- 
ments he  tried  a  reference  electrode  consisting  of  a 
silver  spoon  held  in  the  subject's  mouth.  The  develop- 
ment of  concepts  about  the  EEG  concomitants  of 
grand  mal  epilepsy  had  their  grounding  in  Fischer's 
(300)  recordings  during  experimentally-induced 
seizures  in  dogs. 

The  demonstration  of  the  3  per  sec.  wave-and- 
spike  formation  so  typical  of  the  petit  mal  type  of 
epilepsy  was  the  achievement  of  the  team  of  Lennox, 
Davis  and  the  Git)bses  at  the  Harvard  Medical  .School 
(301).  This  discovery  (which  Berger  came  very  close 
to  making),  together  with  that  of  Grey  Walter  (302) 
published  the  following  year  (1936),  namely  that 
brain  tumors  can  be  located  through  the  skull  by  the 
abnormally  slow  waves  of  their  surrounding  tissue, 
form  the  two  main  foundations  of  clinical  electro- 
encephalography. Altenburg  &  Foerster  (303)  had 
during  a  brain  operation  found  abnormal  potentials 
a.ssociated  with  a  tumor,  but  Walter's  demonstration 
that  neoplasms  could  be  located  by  the  reversal  of 
sign  of  the  slow  waves  recorded  from  the  unopened 
head  and  his  confirmation  that  the  tumor  itself  was 
electrically  silent  made  this  a  practical  clinical  test. 
The  subsequent  expansion  and  development  of 
electroencephalography  is  part  of  the  continuing 
story  of  modern  times  not  yet  history. 

In  the  history  of  electroencephalography  one  other 
figure  should  be  mentioned.  One  year  after  Caton's 
discovery,  Danilewsky,  the  Russian  neurophysiologist, 
noted  the  same  phenomenon  of  oscillating  cortical 
potentials  in  the  absence  of  applied  sensory  stimula- 
tion in  five  dogs  on  which  he  was  experimenting.  He 
did  not  publish  this  at  the  time  and  reported  it  only 


in  retrospect  C304)  as  a  confirmation  of  Caton's 
original  ob.ser\ation.  Danilewsky's  primary  interest 
lay  in  the  autonomic  eff'ects  of  stimulation  of  the  cor- 
tex, such  as  arterial  pressure  changes  (305),  and  in  the 
mechanisms  of  temperature  control  (306),  and  he  was 
active  in  the  design  of  new  instrumentation  for  electro- 
physiological experimentation  (307).  Together  with 
his  brother  (Alexis  Y.  Danilewsky)  he  was  prominent 
among  the  Russian  physiologists  at  the  end  of  the 
nineteenth  century. 

In  the  latter  half  of  the  nineteenth  century,  Russian 
neurophysiology  saw  a  development  that  was  to  in- 
fluence all  future  concepts  about  the  brain  and  be- 
havior. At  this  period  it  was  usual  for  Russian  physiol- 
ogists to  go  to  centers  in  Western  Europe  for  training 
and  experience  under  the  outstanding  teachers  of  the 
time,  and  to  Miiller's  laborator\'  in  1856  came  I.  M. 
.Sechenov.  Secheno\',  later  to  be  known  as  'the  father 
of  Russian  neurophysiologv'  was  then  27  years  old  and 
during  the  next  6  )ears  he  received  training  from  six 
of  the  more  outstanding  physiologists:  Miiller,  du 
Bois-Reymond,  Ludwig,  \'on  Helmholtz,  Bunsen  and 
Claude  Bernard.  The  influence  of  these  leaders  can 
be  traced  in  .Sechenov's  later  thought  and  develop- 
ment. Among  them,  only  one,  Miiller,  retained  even 
a  lingering  trace  of  allegiance  to  the  concept  of  a  vital 
force,  and  with  him  Sechenov-  had  the  least  contact, 
for  Midler  was  at  the  end  of  his  life,  still  lecturing  but 
no  longer  experimenting. 

In  neurophysiology  the  most  influential  of  Seche- 
nov's teachers  were  du  Bois-Reymond  and  Claude 
Bernard.  Sechenov  took  du  Bois-Reymond's  cour.se  in 
animal  electricity  and  in  i860  returned  to  St.  Peters- 
burg with  one  of  his  master's  induction  coil  stimula- 
tors and  a  galvanometer  and  with  them  introduced 
electrophysiology  into  Russian  science.  Two  years  later 
he  was  back  in  Western  Europe,  this  time  in  Claude 
Bernard's  laboratory  in  Paris,  and  it  was  here  that 
the  experiments  were  made  that  were  to  mold  his 
thinking  and  to  suggest  to  him  a  concept  of  brain 
mechanisms  later  to  flower  in  the  hands  of  Pavlov 
into  the  theor\'  that  has  dominated  Russian  neuro- 


300.  Fischer,  Max  H.  Elektrobiologische  ."^uswirkungen  von 
Krampfgiften  am  Zentralnervensystem.  Med.  K/in.  QMu- 
mch^'^g:  15,  1933. 

301.  GiBBS,  F.  A.,  H.  Davis  and  W.  G.  Lennox.  The  EEG  in 
epilepsy  and  in  conditions  of  impaired  consciousness. 
A.  M.  A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychial.  34:  1 133,  1935. 

302.  Walter,  VV.  Grey.  The  location  of  cerebral  tumours  by 
electroencephalography.  Lancet  2:  305,  1936. 

303.  Foerster,  O.  and  H.  Altenburger.  Elektrobiologische 
Vorgange  an  der  menschlichen  Hirnrinde.  Deuliche 
Zlschr.  .Nervenh.  135:  277,  1935. 


304.  D.\nile\vskv,  Vasili  Y.\kovi.evich  (1852- 1 939).  Zur 
Frage  iiber  die  elektromotorischen  Vorgange  im  Gehirn 
als  Ausdruck  seines  Thatig  keitszustandes.  Centralbl. 
Physiol.  5:  I,  1 89 1. 

305.  Danilewsky,  V.  Y.  Experimentelle  Beitrage  zur  Physi- 
ologic des  Gehirns.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  11  :  128,  1875. 

306.  Danilewsky,  V.  Y.  Die  Verbrennungswarme  der  Nah- 
rungsmittel.  Biol,  ^enlralbl.  2:  371,    1882. 

307.  D.^NiLEWsKv,  V.  Y.  A  new  electrical  machine  for  rhyth- 
mically altering  the  strength  of  galvanic  currents  (in 
Russian).  Vralsch.  22,  1883. 


THE  HISTORICAL  DEVELOPMENT  OF  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


53 


FIG.  39.  Ivan  Michailovich  Sechenov  and  his  diagram  illustrating  reflex  arcs  in  the  spinal  cord 
and  brain  of  the  frog,  a-b-c-d  represents  a  spinal  refle.x  arc  with  sensory  (a-A),  central  (b-c)  and 
motor  ((■-(/)  components.  The  reflex  arc  of  the  brain  consists  of  the  sensory  nerve  (O),  the  central 
component  (.V-f)  and  the  motor  efferent  (<:-(/).  P  is  the  region  in  the  brain  stem  where  Sechenov 
concluded  the  inhibitory  apparatus  lay. 


physiology  ever  since,  the  theory  of  conditional  re- 
flexes. 

Sechenov's  experiments  that  proved  so  crucial  to 
his  future  thinking  were  on  the  effect  on  reflex  move- 
ments of  salt  crystals  placed  at  various  levels  of  the 
transected  neuraxis  (308).  His  preparation  (309)  was 
the  decapitated  frog,  a  toe  of  which  he  dipped  into 
acid,  a  procedure  that  had  been  developed  by  Tiirck. 
He  timed  the  interval  between  stimulus  and  onset  of 
withdrawal  of  the  frog's  foot  by  counting  the  beats  of 
a  metronome.  In  this  way  he  got  some  index  of  the 
degree  to  which  application  of  the  salt  crystal  to  the 
brain  stem  slowed  withdrawal.  Sechenov  interpreted 
lengthening  of  withdrawal  time  as  inhibition  of  reflex 
activity.  The  selection  of  a  salt  crystal  as  a  stimulus 
seems  strange  in  the  hands  of  a  pupil  of  du  Bois- 
Reymond's  and  is  reminiscent  of  Marshall  Hall's  use 
of  it  half  a  century  earlier  to  study  depression  and 

308.  Sechenov,  Ivan  Mich.mlovich  (1829-1905).  Physiolo- 
gische  Studien  iiber  die  Hemmungsmechanismus  fur  die  Re- 
flexthdiigkeit  des  Riickenmarks  im  Gehirne  des  Frosches.  Berlin : 
Hirschwald,  1863. 

309.  Sechenov,  I.  M.  Note  sur  les  moderateurs  des  mouve- 
ments  reflexes  dans  le  cerveau  de  la  grenouille.  Acad.  Sc, 
Paris  1863. 


augmentation  of  spinal  reflexes.  Only  later  (310)  did 
.Sechenov  use  electrical  stimulation  in  his  experiments 
on  the  'spontaneous'  variations  of  spinal  cord  poten- 
tials which  he  regarded  as  signs  of  activity  in  the 
spinal  centers.  This  was  the  first  experimental  ap- 
proach towards  a  centrally  exerted  inhibitory  action 
on  skeletal  ('voluntary')  muscle. 

Although  at  this  stage  his  own  experimental  evi- 
dence seemed  slender,  Sechenov  must  have  been 
pondering  its  meaning  in  much  wider  terms,  for  a 
year  later,  on  his  return  to  Rus.sia,  he  published  as  a 
series  of  articles  the  essay  (31 1)  that  proved  to  be  so 
influential  in  Rus.sian  physiology.  This  essay  on  the 
Reflexes  of  the  Brain  was  later  (1866)  published  as  a 
book  after  a  stormy  period  during  which  efforts  were 
made  to  suppress  its  publication  and  censure  its 
author.  This  opposition  was  stirred  by  Sechenov's 
assertion  that  all  higher  brain  function  was  a  material 
reflex  consisting  of  three  sectors — an  afferent  initia- 
tion by  sensory  inflow,  a  central  process  entirely  sub- 

310.  Sechenov,  I.  M.  Galvanische  Erscheinungen  an  dem 
Verlangerten  Marke  des  Frosches.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  27: 
524,  1882. 

311.  Sechenov,  I.  M.  Reflexes  of  Ike  Brain.  Medizinsky  Veslnik, 
1863;  English  translation  in  Sechenov's  Selected  Works. 
Moscow-Leningrad:  State  Publ.  House,  1935,  p.  263. 


54 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


ject  to  physical  laws  and  an  efferent  component  result- 
ing in  a  muscular  movement.  All  reactions,  however 
they  might  be  described  in  common  parlance  as 
pleasure,  fear,  distress  or  other  descriptive  terms  were, 
according  to  him,  in  essence  muscular  in  expression. 
During  the  passage  of  the  inflow  through  the  central 
portion  of  the  arc  there  could  either  be  excitation 
which  would  augment  the  reflex  motor  response  (as 
in  so-called  emotional  states)  or  inhibition  which 
would  decrease  the  reflex  muscular  movement,  the 
resultant  beine;  'rational'  controlled  behavior.  It  is 
interesting  that  Sechenov  conceived  that  inhibition 
could  be  learned  and  that  with  maturity  an  increase 
in  the  degree  of  inhibition  exerted  was  achieved. 

Thus,  according  to  Sechenov,  all  human  behavior 
was  a  balance  between  inhibition  and  excitation 
operating  mechanically  at  the  central  link  of  the  re- 
flex arc.  A  so-called  'willed'  movement  according  to 
him  only  apparently  lacked  the  first  component  of  the 
arc,  its  afferent  inflow  being  material  memory  traces 
left  by  external  stimuli  in  the  past.  It  was  in  elaborat- 
ing this  part  of  his  theory  that  Sechenov  approached 
the  concept  of  the  conditional  reflex,  for  he  postulated 
that  the  memory  trace  of  a  past  sensory  experience 
could  be  evoked  by  the  recurrence  of  any  fraction  of 
it  even  if  this  fraction  were  quite  insignificant  and 
unrelated  in  its  apparent  meaning.  This  is  essentially 
the  principle  underlying  the  formulation  of  the  condi- 
tional reflex  theory,  namely  the  potency  of  an  indiffer- 
ent external  stimulus  provided  it  is  repeatedly  time- 
locked  to  the  original  experience.  One  further  point 
should  be  noted  in  this  early  attempt  to  relate  mental 
processes  to  brain  physiology.  Sechenov  believed  that 
man  had  the  special  faculty  of  increasing  the  degree 
of  inhibition  exerted  at  the  central  link  until  a  level 
of  total  inhibition  of  the  efferent  discharge  was 
reached,  and  he  held  that  thought  was  an  example  of 
this  condition. 

Although  terms  such  as  'cerebral  reflexes'  and 
'psychical  reflexes'  abound  in  the  nineteenth  century 
literature,  they  were  mostly  used  by  psychologists 
to  describe  automatisms.  At  this  period  only  a  few 
writers  had  broached  the  problem  of  explaining 
mental  processes  in  physiological  terms.  Thomas  Lay- 
cock  C312),  whose  belief  in  cortical  localization  no 
doubt  influenced  his  pupil  Hughlings  Jackson,  wrote 
in  1845  a  paper  On  the  reflex  function  of  the  brain.  In  this 
he  stated  his  belief  that  "the  brain  although  the  organ 
of  consciousness,  was  subject  to  the  laws  of  reflex  ac- 
tion, and  that  in  this  respect  it  did  not  differ  from  other 

312.   Laycock,  Thomas  (1812-1876).   On  the  reflex  function 
of  the  brain.  Brit.  &  For.  Med.  Rev.  19:  298,  1845. 


ganglia  of  the  nervous  system."  He  too  envisaged  a 
three-component  arc,  the  central  link  in  the  brain 
being  one  where  'ideagenous'  changes  took  place  that 
influenced  the  motor  output.  He  came  close  to  antic- 
ipating one  of  Sechenov's  postulates  by  stating  that 
the  actual  sensory  impression  of  an  object  or  the  mere 
idea  of  it  could  evoke  the  same  'ideagenous'  change 
in  the  brain  and  result  in  a  similar  reflex  motor  effect. 
So  firmly  did  Laycock  believe  in  the  neuronal  basis 
of  ideas  that  he  calculated  how  many  there  could  be 
to  the  square  inch  of  grey  matter  (the  answer  was 
8000)  and  argued  that  "as  there  must  be  an  immense 
number  of  square  inches  of  surface  in  the  grey  matter 
extended  through  the  cerebrospinal  axis  of  man,  there 
is  space  sufficient  for  millions."  We  find  echoes  of  this 
kind  of  calculation  in  some  of  today's  conjectures 
about  the  number  of  possible  interconnections  in  the 
brain. 

Laycock  did  not  test  his  hypotheses  by  experiment 
though  he  argued  from  a  basis  of  clinical  observation, 
for  he  said  "an  experiment  is  occasionally  made  by 
nature."  There  is  no  evidence  that  Sechenov  was 
aware  of  Laycock's  ideas,  although  he  was  influenced 
by  the  writings  of  two  other  nonexperimentalists, 
Herbert  Spencer  (313)  and  George  Henry  Lewes 
(314).  These  two  men,  united  through  their  relation- 
ships with  George  Eliot,  were  influential  not  only  on 
Sechenov  but  on  Pavlov.  Their  writings,  now  largely 
unread,  were  translated  into  Russian  almost  immedi- 
ately after  publication  and  were  everywhere  highly 
regarded.  .Spencer's  work  was  an  argument  for  cortical 
representation  of  mental  function,  and  Hughlings 
Jackson  was  one  who  expressed  indebtedness  to  him. 
Spencer  based  much  of  his  argument  on  comparative 
evolution  though  he  was  writing  4  years  before  the 
publication  of  the  Origin  of  the  Species  by  Darwin 
(315),  another  writer  whose  books  were  extremely 
influential  on  Ru.ssian  thought.  Spencer  stressed 
localization  of  mental  processes,  saying  that  "whoever 
calmly  considers  the  question  cannot  long  resist  the 
conviction  that  different  parts  of  the  brain  must  in 
some  way  or  other  suhserve  different  kinds  of  mental 
action."  When  we  find  in  his  Autobiography  (316)  that 

313.  Spencer,  Herbert  (1820-1903).  Principles  of  Psychology. 
1855.  2  vol. 

314.  Lewes,  George  Henry  (18 17-1 878).  The  Physiology  of  the 
Common  Life.  London:  Blackwood,  1859.  2  \'ol. 

315.  Darwin,  Charles  Robert  (1809-1882).  On  the  Origin  of 
Species  by  means  of  .Kaiural  Selection  or  the  Preservation  of 
Favoured  Races  in  the  Struggle  for  Life.  London:  John 
Murray,  1859. 

316.  Spencer,  H.  .In  Autobiography.  London:  Williams  & 
Norgate,  1904.  2  vol. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


FIG.  40.  Ivan  Petrovich  Pavlov  (reproduced  from  Babkin's  Pavlov,  A  &'ogra/)A>  by  permission  of  the 
publishers.  University  of  Chicago  Press).  On  the  right  Pavlov  watching  an  experiment  (Sovfoto 
* 3' 9573  Moscow  USSR). 


he  had  the  bumps  on  his  head  read  by  a  phrenologist 
(with  flattering  interpretations)^'  we  perceive  a  deri- 
vation of  his  ideas  from  Gall  and  Spurzheim.  Spencer 
became  hypochondriacal  about  his  own  head,  believing 
it  to  have  an  inadequate  blood  supply.  To  improve 
the  circulation  he  exercised  at  rowing  and  at  racquets 
in  15  mill,  spurts,  dictating  his  books  in  the  intervals 
between  exertions.  His  friend,  Lewes  (3 1 7)  in  his 
Physical  Basis  of  Mind  was  doubtful  about  the  localiza- 
tion of  the  various  mental  processes  but  convinced  of 
their  physiological  nature. 

Pavlov,  the  towering  figure  of  Russian  neurophysiol- 
ogy, repeatedly  throughout  his  life  stressed  his  in- 
debtedness to  Sechenov'"  and  to  Lewes^'  (whose  book 
on  physiology  he  read  when  a  schoolboy).  The 
influence  of  these  men,  one  too  little  known  outside 
Russia,  one  almost  forgotten,  was  so  great  that  they 
feature  not  only  in  the  scientific  writings  of  the  times 
but  in  Russian  fiction.  Turgeniev  is  said  to  have 
taken  Sechenov  as  his  model  for  the  science  student, 
Bazarov,  in  Fathers  and  Sons  and  Dostoievsky  cited 
the  reading  of  Lewes'  book  as  a  sign  of  education  in 
the  wife  of  a  drunk  in  Crime  and  Punishment. 

Pavlov  dated  his  interest  in  the  digestive  system 
(318)  from  reading  Lewes,  an  interest  that  was  to 
occupy  the  first  25  years  of  his  working  life  and  to  win 
for  him  the  Nobel  Prize.  And  it  was  a  feature  of  the 
digestive  system,  the  salivary  apparatus,  that  was  to 
be  drawn  by  him  into  the  work  suggested  by  Sech- 

317.  Lev^^s,  G.  H.  The  Physical  Basis  of  Mind.  Boston,  1877. 

318.  Pavlov,  I.  P.  (1B49-1936).  Lectures  on  the  Work  of  the 
Principal  Digestive  Glands  (in  Russian).  St.  Petersburg: 
Kushnerev,  1897;  translated  into  English  by  W.  H. 
Thompson.  London:  Griffin,  190J. 


enov's  theories  of  30  years  before.  Fundamental  in 
Pavlov's  thinking  (319)  was  the  concept  of  temporary 
connections  established  in  the  cortex  by  the  repetition 
of  external  stimuli  linked  only  by  a  constant  time 
interval,  although  one  gets  the  impression  that  he 
thought  more  in  terms  of  influence  than  of  specific 
neuronal  connections.  Thus,  for  example,  in  his 
classical  experiment,  the  repeated  sound  of  a  metro- 
nome, at  a  fixed  interval  before  food  was  made  avail- 
able to  his  experimental  dogs,  caused  salivation  to 
begin  with  shorter  and  shorter  latency  and  at  an 
increasing  rate.  Later  more  complex  situations  were 
developed  as  laboratory  procedures,  and  this  type  of 
reflex  was  used  for  mapping  the  response  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  to  various  sensory  inputs,  Pavlov 
(319)  naming  the  areas  as  "analyzers'  for  the  various 
modalities. 

The   instability   and    temporary   character   of  the 
conditioned  reflex  in  contrast  to  that  of  the  inborn 

319.  Pavlov,  I.  P.  Lectures  on  Conditioned  Reflexes,  English 
translation  by  \V.  H.  Gantt.  New  York:  Internat.  Pub., 
1928. 


"'The  opening  sentence  of  the  phrenologist's  report  read: 
"Such  a  head  ought  to  be  in  the  Church."  When  we  seek  the 
basis  for  this  statement  in  the  itemized  score  for  Spencer's 
bumps,  we  find  both  Firmness  and  Self-esteem  'very  large;' 
Language  'rather  full,'  and  Wit  and  Amativeness  only  'moder- 
ate.' 

'°  See  Shaternikov,  M.  N.  The  life  of  I.  M.  Sechenov.  In : 
Sechenov,  Selected  Works.  Moscow-Leningrad:  State  Publ.  House, 

■935- 

^' See  Babkin,  B.  P.  Pavlov.  Chicago:  Univ.  Chicago  Press, 
1949,  p.  214. 


56 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


unconditioned  reflexes  serving  instinctual  movements 
for  preservation  of  life  led  to  Pavlov's  ideas  of  cortical 
inhibition  and  its  relationship  to  sleep  and  hypnosis. 
Pavlov  distinguished  between  natural  conditional 
reflexes  learned  in  early  life  and  the  artificially 
conditional  reflexes  of  the  laboratory.  Among  the 
first  he  classed  the  connections  formed  in  infancy 
between  the  smell  or  sight  of  food  and  the  salivary 
response.  This  observation  goes  back  many  centuries 
and  is  well  described  by  Whytt  (i8i),  who  like  Lay- 
cock  after  him,  recognized  that  the  'idea'  could  be  as 
powerful  a  stimulus  as  the  sensory  impression. 

Pavlov  had  in  his  youth  been  a  student  of  Ludwig 
and  of  Heidenhain;  from  the  former  he  had  brought 
the  insistence  on  a  physical  basis  for  all  biological 
processes  and  from  the  latter  an  interest  in  secretory 
mechanisms  and  the  phenomena  of  hypnosis.  The 
fertility  of  Pavlov's  ideas  and  his  indefatigable  energy 
drew  to  him  an  enthusiastic  school  of  workers  and  b\' 
the  1920's  he  had  a  large  team  working  under  him  on 
the  many  features  of  establishment,  reinforcement, 
extinction  and  inhibition  of  conditional  reflexes.  He 
was  a  well-loved  teacher,  though  a  man  of  fiery 
temperament.  Sherrington  has  left  a  vignette  of  him 
at  the  age  of  65  describing  him  as  "overflowing  with 
energy,  although  an  elderly  man;  he  was  spare  in 
figure  and  alert  and  humourous  in  manner."  Even 
at  the  end  of  a  long  working  day  on  encountering  a 
stairway  he  "ran  up  it  rather  than  walked."  Sherring- 
ton came  away  from  this  visit,  made  in  191 4,  with  a 
great  enthusiasm  for  the  leader  of  Russian  neuro- 
physiology C320). 

Pavlov's  ideas  of  the  reflex  became  more  diffuse 
and  more  nebulous  as  he  grew  older.  Experiments  to 
test  the  modes  of  behavior  of  animals  to  conditioning 
stimuli  were  less  difficult  to  design  than  ones  to  test 
the  hypothesis  advanced  to  explain  them.  Temporary 
neuronal  connections  in  the  cortex  proved  easier  to 
postulate  than  to  prove.  Pavlov's  own  attempts  were 
with  decorticate  preparations  (a  technique  that  had 
been  u.sed  before  him  by  Sechenov)  and  it  is  only  in 
recent  times  that  the  electrophysiologist's  tools  have 
been  applied  to  this  problem. 

320.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Marginalia.  In:  Science,  Medicine  and 
History.  Essays  in  Honour  of  C.  Singer,  edited  by  E.  A.  Un- 
derwood. London:  Oxford,  1953. 


As  the  second  half  of  the  twentieth  century  unfolds 
the  neurophysiologist  in  his  search  for  brain  mech- 
anisms continues  to  use  the  three  main  categories  of 
experimental  procedure:  anatomical,  ablative  and 
electrical.  It  is  the  great  advance  in  electrical  stimula- 
tion and  recording  that  marks  this  era  of  investigation 
from  its  predecessors,  although  it  is  only  through 
knowledge  from  all  sources  that  progress  can  be 
achieved  in  an  understanding  of  the  brain. 

Neurophysiology  came  into  being  as  a  specialized 
branch  of  endeavor  when  the  nervous  system  no 
longer  had  to  compete  with  the  humors  and  with  the 
blood  as  the  principal  coordinator  of  the  body.  With 
the  recognition  that  sensation  and  motion  were  medi- 
ated by  the  nerves  their  position  became  unassailable, 
for  movement  was  regarded  as  the  sign  of  life.  Slowly 
the  concept  of  neural  organization  began  to  be  pieced 
together  and  levels  of  integration  were  postulated,  in 
the  spinal  cord,  in  the  cortex  and  in  the  deeper  struc- 
tures of  the  brain.  The  period  of  analysis  of  the  func- 
tion of  each  structural  unit,  of  each  sector  of  the 
nervous  system,  was  followed  b\'  a  shift  of  emphasis 
towards  a  synthetic  consideration  of  neural  activity. 
The  search  began  for  the  physiological  mechanisms 
of  mental  processes,  of  consciousness,  of  memory — all 
terms  and  concepts  that  had  belonged  to  another  do- 
main of  thought.  In  the  neurophysiolog\'  of  today  we 
find  both  angles  of  approach,  ranging  from  analysis 
of  the  intimate  physicochemicai  basis  of  nervous 
structure  and  dynamics  to  the  synthesis  of  action  that 
we  call  behavior  of  the  organism. 

The  writer  expresses  her  great  indebtedness  to  the  authors 
of  many  articles  and  books  not  listed  in  the  abridged  bibli- 
ography that  follows.  She  adds  her  thanks  to  those  who  have 
sent  her  material  in  correspondence,  and  in  particular  would 
mention  appreciatively:  Dr.  Maria  Rooseboom  for  the  use  of 
material  and  microfilms  from  the  National  Museum  for  the 
History  of  .Science  at  Leiden;  Dr.  Palle  Birkelund,  Director  of 
the  Danish  Royal  Library;  Dr.  .\uguste  Tournay  for  a  photo- 
stat copy  of  Pourfour  du  Petit's  Letters,  the  Institution  of  Elec- 
trical Engineers  and  Miss  Helen  G.  Thompson  for  access  to 
material  collected  by  Silvanus  P.  Thompson  on  Gilberd;  Miss 
Anne  Caton  for  family  photographs  and  material  from  the 
diaries  of  Richard  Caton;  Dr.  Andrei  Jus  of  Pruszkov  for  photo- 
stats of  Adolf  Beck's  doctoral  thesis;  and  F.  Czubalski  of  War- 
saw for  information  about  Beck's  works.  For  details  of  Beck's 
life  the  writer  expresses  warm  appreciation  to  his  daughter, 
Mme.  Jadwiga  Zahrzewska. 


A  SHORT  LIST  OF  SECONDARY  SOURCES 

Space  does  not  permit  the  listing  of  all  the  articles  to  whose 
authors  the  writer  is  indebted  for  information.  The  following 
books  have  been  selected  for  the  special  interest  they  may 
have  for  the  physiologist.  Where  possible,  works  in  the  English 
language  have  been  chosen. 


Bence  Jones,  H.  On  Animal  Electricity.  London:  Churchill,  1852. 
Bettmann,   O.    L.   .4   Pictorial  History  of  .Medicine.   Springfield: 

Thomas,  1956. 
Boring,  E.  G.  A  History  of  Experimental  Psychology.  New  \'ork: 

.\ppleton,  1 929. 


THE    HISTORICAL    DEVELOPMENT    OF    NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


57 


BosTOCK,  J.  Sketch  of  the  Hutory  nf  Medicine  from   its   Origin  to 

the  Commencement  of  the  .\ineteenlh  Century.  London:  Sherwood, 

Gilbert  and  Piper,  1835. 
Brown,   G.    B.   Science,   its   Method  and  1/1    Philosophy.    London: 

Allen  &  Unwin,  1951 
Canguilhem,    G.    La    Formation    du    Concept    de    Reflexe.    Paris: 

Presses  univ.  France,  1955. 
Castiglioni,   a.   Italian   Medicine.    New   York:   Hoeber,    1932, 

Clio  Medica  Series,  vol.  6. 
Castiglioni,   A.   .-i   History  of  Medicine  (2nd  ed.)-   New  York: 

Knopf,  1947. 
CoMRIE,  J.  D.  A  History  of  Scottish  Medicine.  London:  Wellcome 

Historical  Medical  Museum,  1932. 
Corner,  G.  \V.  Anatomy.  New  York :  Hoeber,  1 930,  Clio  Medica 

Series,  vol.  3. 
Cooke,  J.  A    Treatise  on  Nervous  Diseases.  Boston:  Wells  and 

Lilly,  1824.  (Previously  published  in  England.) 
Dampier,  W.  C.  a  History  of  Science.  Cambridge:  Cambridge, 

1946. 
Dana,  C.  Textbook  of  Nervous  Diseases.  New  York :  Wm.  Wood, 

1925.  (Includes  a  chapter  by  F.  H.  Garrison  on  the  history 

of  neurology.) 
D.^REMBERG,    C.    Essai   sur   la   determination    et   ies   caracthes   des 

periodes  de  I'histoire  de  la  medecine.  Gaz.  med.  Paris,  1850. 
Fearing,  F.  Reflex  Action.  Baltimore:  Williams  &  Wilkins,  1930. 
Foster,  M.    Textbook  of  Physiology  (1st  American  ed.),  edited 

by  E.  T.  Reichert.  Philadelphia:  H.  C.  Lea's  son  and  Co., 

1880.  (1st  English  ed.,  1876.) 
Foster,   M.   Lectures   on   the  History  of  Physiology.    Cambridge: 

Cambridge,  igoi. 
Fr.^nki.in,   K.   a   Short  History  of  Physiology.    London:   Staples, 

1949- 
Fulton,    J.    F.    Muscular    Contraction   and  the   Reflex   Control   of 

Movement.  Baltimore:  Williams  &  Wilkins,  1926. 

Fulton,  J.  F.  Selected  Readings  in  the  History  of  Physiology.  Spring- 
field:  Thomas,  1930. 

Fulton,  J.  F.  Physiology.  New  York:  Hoeber,  1931,  Clio  Medica 
Series,  vol.  5. 

Fulton,  J.  F.  Physiology  of  the  Nervous  System  (3rd  ed.).  New 
York :  O.xford,  1 949. 

Garrison,  F.  H.  An  Introduction  to  the  History  of  Medicine.  Phila- 
delphia:  Saunders,  1929. 

Hall,  A.  R.  The  Scientific  Revolution  ijou-iSuo.  London: 
Longmans,  1954. 

Hamilton,  W.  The  History  of  Medicine,  Surgery  and  Anatomy  from 
the  Creation  of  the  World,  to  the  Commencement  of  the  Nineteenth 
Century.  London:  Colburn  and  Bentley,  1831. 

Lenard,  p.  Great  Men  of  Science,  translated  by  H.  S.  Hatfield. 
New  York:  Macmillan,  1933. 

M.«.jor,  R.  --1  History  of  Medicine.  Springfield:  Thomas,  1954. 

Morgan,  C.  E.  Electro-Physiology  and  Therapeutics.  New  York: 
Wood,  1868. 


Morton,  L.  T.    and  F.  H.  Garrison.   A  Medical  Bibliography 

(2nd  ed.).  London:  Grafton,  1954. 
Nordenskiold,  E.  History  of  Biology,  translated  by  L.  B.  Eyre. 

New  York:  Tudor,   1935. 
Pettigrev\',  T.  J.  Medical  Portrait  Gallery.  Biographical  Memoirs 

of  the  most  Celebrated  Physicians,  Surgeons,  etc.  London:  Fisher, 

1872.  3  vol. 
PoTAMiN   (Brother)   and  J.  J.   Walsh.    Makers  of  Electricity. 

New  York:  Fordham  Univ.  Press,  1909. 
Renouard,  p.  V.  History  of  Medicine  from  its  Origin  to  the  .\ine- 

teenth  Century,  translated  by  C.   G.   Comegys.   Philadelphia: 

Lippincott,  1856.  2  vol. 

Rothschuh,  K.   E.  Geschichte  der  Physiologic.  Berlin :  Springer, 

1953 
Russell,   T.   R.    The  History  of   Heroes  of  the  Art  of  Medicine. 

London:  Murray,  1861. 
ScHAFER,  E.  A.   Textbook  of  Physiology.  Edinburgh  and  London: 

Y.  J.  Pentland,  1898,  vol.  i ;  1900,  vol.  2. 
Shryock,    R.    H.    The   Development   of  Modern    Medicine.    New 

York:  Knopf,  1947. 
Singer,  C.  .-1  Short  History  of  Medicine.  New  Y'ork:  Oxford,  1928. 
Singer,  C.  J.  Essays  on  the  History  of  Medicine.  London:  Oxford, 

■9^4- 
Singer,  C.  J.   The  Evolution  of  Anatomy;  a  Short  History  of  Ana- 
tomical and  Physiological  Discovery  to  Harvey.   London :   Paul, 

Trench,  Trubner,  1925. 
Soury,  J.  Le  Systhne  nerveux  central.  .Structure  el  Fonctions.  Paris: 

Carre  et  Naud,  1899. 
Sprengel,  K.  Histoire  de  la  Medecine,  translated  and  abridged 

from    the   2nd   German   ed.    by    A.   J.    L.   Jourdan.    Paris: 

Deterville,  1792-1803.  2  vol. 
Stirling,  W.  Some  Apostles  of  Physiology.   London:  Waterlow, 

1902. 
Sudhoff,  K.  Essays  in  the  History  of  Medicine,  English  translation 

edited  by  F.  H.  Garrison.  New  York :  Medical  Life  Press, 

1926. 
Whewell,  W.  History  of  the  Inductive  Sciences.   London:  Parker 

1837-  3  ™1- 

Whittaker,  E.  T.  History  of  the  Theories  of  Aether  and  Elec- 
tricity from  the  Age  of  Descartes  to  the  Close  of  the  Nineteenth 
Century.  London  and  New  York:  Longmans,  Green  and  Co., 
igio,  revi.sed  1951,  second  volume  added  1953. 

Wightman,  W.  p.  D.  The  Growth  of  Scientific  Ideas.  New  Haven: 
Yale  Univ.  Press,  1951. 

Wilkinson,  C.  H.  Elements  of  Galvanism  in  Theory  and  Practice. 
London:  Murray,  1804. 

Wolf,  A.  A  History  of  Science,  Technology  and  Philosophy  in  the 
1 6th  and  lyth  Centuries  (2nd  ed.).  London:  .-\llen  and  L'nwin, 

I950- 
Wolf,  A.  A  History  of  Science,    Technology  and  Philosophy  in  the 
1 8th  Century  (2nd  ed.).  London:  .\llen  and  Unwin,  1952. 


BIOGRAPHIES 

For  each  of  the  following  scientists  one  biographical  study 
only  has  been  listed.  Again  the  choice  has  been  made  on  the 
grounds  of  interest  to  the  physiologist  and,  where  possible,  text 
in  the  English  language. 

Aristotle.  Taylor,  A.  E.  Aristotle.  London:  Nelson,  1943. 
Bacon,    Francis   (i 561-1626).    Farrington,    B.    Francis    Bacon, 
Philosopher  of  Industrial  Science.  New  York:  Schuman,   1949. 


Baglivi,  Giorgio  (1668-1707).  Stenn,  F.  Giorgio  Baglivi.  Ann. 

Med.  Hist.  (3rd  ser.)  3:  183,  1941. 
Bell,  Charles  (i  774-1 842).  Pichot,  A.  The  Life  and  Labours  of 

Sir  Charles  Bell.  London:  Bentley,  1880. 
Berger,   Hans  (1873-1942).   Ginzberg,   R.   Three  years  with 

Hans  Berger.  .\  contribution  to  his  biography.  J.  Hist.  Med. 

4:  361,  1949- 
Bernard,  Cl.aude  (1813-1878).  Olmsted,  J.  M.  D.  and  E.  H. 


58 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Olmsted.    Claude    Bernard,    Physiologist,    and   the    Experimental 

Method  in  Medicine.  New  York:  Schuman,  1952. 
BicHAT,  Marie  Francois  Xavier  (1771-1802).   Busquet,   P. 

Les  Biographies  Medicates  I:  37,  1927. 
BoERHAAVE,  HERMANN  (1668-1738).  Burton,  W.  An  Account  of 

the  Life  and  Writings  of  Hermann  Boerhaave.  London:  Lintot, 

1743- 
CoTUGNO,   DoMENico   (1736-1822).    LevinsoH,   A.    Domenico 

Cotugno.  Ann.  Med.  Hist.  8:  i,  1936. 
DA  Vinci,  Leonardo  (1452-1519).  O'Malley,  C.  D.  and  J. 

B.  deC.  M.  Saunders.  Leonardo  da  Vinci  on  the  Human  Body. 

New  York:  Schuman,  1952. 
Descartes,  Rene  (1596- 1650).  Haldane,  E.    The  Life  of  Rene 

Descartes.  London,  1905. 
Fernel,  Jean  (1497-1558).  Sherrington,  C.  S.    The  Endeavour 

of  Jean  Fernel.  Cambridge:  Cambridge,  1946. 
FoNTANA,  Felice  (1730-1805).  Marchand,  J.  F.  and  H.  E. 

Hoff.  Felice  Fontana.  The  Laws  of  Irritability.  J.  Hist.  Med. 

10:  197,  302,  399,  1955. 
Galen   (130-200).   Sarton,   G.   Galen   of  Pergamon.    Lawrence: 

Lfniv.  Kansas  Press,  1954. 
Gall,  Franz  Joseph  (1758-1828).  Temkin,  O.  Gall  and  the 

phrenological  movement.  Bull.  Hist.  Med.  21 :  275,  1947. 
Galvani,  Alovsius  (i  737-1 798).  Fulton,  J.  F.  and  H.  Gush- 
ing. A  biographical  sketch  of  the  Galvani  and  Aldini  writings 

on  animal  electricity.  Ann.  Sci.  i :  239,  1936. 
GiLBERD,    William   (1540   or    1544- 1603).    Waldron,    F.    G. 

Biographical  Mirrour.  London :  Harding,  1 795. 
Hales,  Stephen  (1677-1761).  Burget,  G.  E.  Stephen  Hales, 

1677-1761.  Ann.  Med.  Hist.  7:  109,  1925. 
Hall,   Marshall   (1790- 1857).    Hall,    Charlotte.    Memoirs  of 

Marshall  Hall.  London,  1861. 
Harvey,  William  (1578-1657).  Chauvois,  L.   William  Harvey. 

His    Life   and    Times.-   his   Discoveries:   his   Methods.    London: 

Hutchinson,  1957. 
Horslev,  Victor  Alexander  Haden  (1857-1916).  Paget,  S. 

Sir  Victor  Horsley,  a  Study  of  his  Life  and   Work.  New  York: 

Harcourt  Brace,  1920. 
Hunter,    John  (i 728-1 793).  Paget,  S.  John  Hunter.  London: 

Fisher  Unwin,  1897. 
LiNACRE,   Thomas   (1460-1524).  Johnson,  J.   N.    The  Life  of 

Thomas  Linacre.  London,  1835. 
LuDWiG,   Carl  Friedrich  Wilhelm  (i 816-1 895).   Lombard, 

W.  P.  The  Life  and  Work  of  Carl  Ludwig.  Science  44:  363, 

1916. 
M.agendie,  Francois  (1783-1855).  Olmsted,  J.  M.  D.  Francois 

Magendie.  New  York:  Schuman,  1944. 
Monro,  .^LEXANDER    (1697-1762)    and   Monro,   .Alexander 


Secundus  (1733-1817).  Inglis,  J.  A.  The  Monros  of  Auchin- 
bowie.  Edinburgh,  1911. 

MOller,  Johannes  (1801-1858).  Haberling,  W.  Johannes  .Mai- 
ler. Leipzig,  1924. 

Nollet,  Jean  Antoine.  Torlais,  J.  Un  Physicien  au  Steele  des 
Lumieres:  Abbe  JVollet.  Paris:  Siprico,  1954. 

Oersted,  Hans  Christian  (i  770-1851).  Stauflfer,  R.  C. 
Speculation  and  experiment  in  the  background  of  Oersted's 
discovery  of  electromagnetism.  Isis  48:  33,  1957. 

Pavlov,  Ivan  Petrovich  (1849-1936).  Babkin,  B.  P.  Pavlov. 
Chicago:  Univ.  Chicago  Press,  1949. 

Prochaska,  Jiri  (1749-1820).  Laycock,  T.  Introduction. 
To:  The  Principles  of  Physiology.  London:  Sydenham  .Society, 
1851,  p.  ix. 

Ramon  y  Cajal,  Santiago  (1852-1934).  Cannon,  D.  Explorer 
of  the  Human  Brain.  New  York:  Schuman,  1949. 

Sechenov,  Ivan  Mihailovich  (1829-1905).  Shaternikov,  M. 
N.  The  life  of  I.  M.  Sechenov  (in  English).  In:  Sechenov, 
Selected  Works.  Moscow-Leningrad:  State  Publ.  House,  1935. 

Stahl,  Georg  Ernst  (1660-1734).  Metzger,  H.  Newton,  Stahl, 
Boerhaave  et  la  doctrine  chimique.  Paris:  Alcan,  1930. 

Stensen,  Nicholas  (1638-1686).  .Nicolaus  Steno  and  His  Indice, 
edited  by  G.  Scherz.  Copenhagen:  Munksgaard,  1958. 

Unzer,  Johann  August  (1727-1799).  Laycock,  T.  Introduc- 
tion. To:  The  Principles  of  Physiology.  London:  Sydenham 
Society,  1851,  p.  i. 

VAN  Leeuwenhoek,  .Antonj  (1672-1723).  Dobell,  C.  Antony 
van  Leeuwenhoek  and  his  "Little  Animals."  New  York:  Harcourt 
Brace,  1932. 

Vesalius,  .■\ndreas  (1514-1564).  Gushing,  H.  A  Bio-bibliog- 
raphy of  Andreas  Vesalius.  New  York:  .Schuman,  1943. 

Volta,  Alessandro  (1745-1827).  Cohen,  I.  B.  Introduction. 
To:  Galvani's  Commentary,  English  translation  by  M.  G. 
Foley.  Norwalk:  Burndy  Library,  1954. 

von  Guericke,  Otto  (1602-1686).  Hoffmann,  F.  W.  Otto  von 
Guericke.  Magdeburg,  1874. 

VON  Haller,  Albrecht  (1708-1777).  Klotz,  O.  Albrecht  von 
Haller  1708-1777.  Ann.  .Med.  Hist.  8:  10,  1936.  Also:  Hem- 
meter,  J.  C.  Albrecht  von  Haller,  his  scientific,  literary  and 
poetic  activity.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  19:  65,  1908. 

VON  Helmholtz,  Hermann  Ludvvig  Ferdinand  (1821-1894). 
McKendrick,  ].  G.  H.  L.  F.  von  Helmholtz-  London:  Unwin, 

1899- 
VON  Humboldt,  Frederick  .'\lexander  (i  769-1859).  de  Terra, 

H.   The  Life  and  Times  of  Alexander  von  Humboldt.  New  York: 

Knopf,  1955. 
Whytt,  Robert  (1714-1766).  Seller,  W.  Memoir  of  the  Life  and 

Writings  of  Robert  Whytt,  .\L  D.  Edinburgh:  Neill,  1862. 
Willis,   Thomas  (1621-1675).    Miller,   W.   S.   Thomas   Willis 

(1621-1675).  Bull.  Soc.  Med.  Hist.  Chicago  3:  215,   1923. 


CHAPTER    II 


Neuron  physiology — introduction 


J.    C.    ECCLES     I     Department  of  Physiology^  Australian  National  University, Canberra,  Australia 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Morphological  Features  of  the  Neuron 

Physiological  Properties  of  Surface  Membranes  of  Neurons 

Transmission  Between  Neurons 

Excitatory  Synaptic  Action 

Inhibitory  Synaptic  Action 

Factors  Controlling  Impulse  Generation 

Central  Inhibitory  Pathways 

Inhibitory  and  Excitatory  Transmitter  Substances 


MORPHOLOGICAL  FEATURES  OF  THE  NEURON 

THE  CONCEPT  that  the  nervous  system  is  composed  of 
discrete  units  or  nerve  cells  was  first  proposed  in 
1886-7  tiy  His  and  Forel,  later  it  was  strongly  sup- 
ported by  van  Gehuchten  and  Cajal,  and  finally  in 
1 89 1  it  was  given  an  appropriate  nomenclature, 
'  neuron'  and  '  neuron-theory',  by  Waldeyer.  Al- 
though all  the  great  neurohistologists  of  that  classical 
era  were  ranged  for  or  against  the  neuron  theory,  it 
was  pre-eminently  the  achievement  of  Cajal  to  estab- 
lish the  fact  that  the  functional  connections  between 
individual  nerve  cells,  or  neurons,  are  effected  by 
close  contacts  and  not  by  continuity  in  a  syncytial 
network,  as  was  proposed  in  the  rival  reticular  theory 
of  Gerlach  and  Golgi.  Appropriately,  Cajal's  last 
great  contribution  (11)  was  devoted  to  a  critical  sur- 
vey of  the  evidence  for  and  against  the  neuron  theory, 
which  has  not  been  seriously  challenged  since  that 
time,  at  least  for  the  vertebrate  nervous  system. 

Neurons  have  the  most  diverse  forms,  yet  there  are 
certain  features  that  are  common  to  all.  The  nucleus 
always  lies  in  an  expanded  part,  the  soma  or  cell 
body,  from  which  the  axon  takes  origin  and  often  runs 
for  long  distances  before  breaking  up  into  the  synaptic 


terminals  that  make  contact  either  with  other  neurons 
or  with  effector  cells  such  as  muscles,  glands  or  elec- 
tric organs.  Under  physiological  conditions  of  opera- 
tion, axons  (with  the  exception  of  primary  afferent 
axons)  transmit  impulses  only  in  the  centrifugal 
direction  and  thus  constitute  the  effector  apparatus  of 
the  nerve  cell.  The  different  types  of  nerve  cells  show 
much  more  variation  in  their  other  branches,  the 
dendrites,  which  normally  share  with  the  soma  the 
receptive  function  for  the  nerve  cell.  Pyramidal  cells 
of  the  cerebral  cortex  and  the  Purkinje  cells  of  the 
cerebellum  have  the  most  extensively  branched  den- 
drites, but  most  neurons  of  the  central  nervous  system 
have  fairly  elaborate  dendritic  structures.  By  contrast, 
in  the  dorsal  root  ganglion  cells  the  receptive  structure 
is  remotely  located  in  the  receptor  organs  which  are 
connected  to  the  soma  by  a  long  axon-like  fiber  that 
normally  conducts  in  the  centripetal  direction,  and 
which  we  may  call  the  primary  afferent  axon. 

\'ery  great  functional  significance  is  attached  to  the 
surface  membrane  of  the  neuron.  This  membrane 
must  not  be  confused  with  the  fibrous,  glial  and 
myelin  structures  which  contribute  a  sheath  to 
neurons,  providing  them  with  mechanical  strength 
and  electrical  insulation.  Until  the  advent  of  electron- 
microscopy  the  surface  membrane  had  not  been  ob- 
served directly;  yet  it  was  an  essential  postulate  in 
explanations  of  the  electrical  properties  of  the  surface 
of  the  neuron  and  of  the  manner  in  which  its  interior 
was  maintained  at  a  very  different  compo.sition  from 
the  exterior,  particularly  in  respect  to  such  ionic 
species  as  sodium,  potassium  and  chloride.  It  also 
provided  a  structural  basis  for  explaining  such  funda- 
mental processes  as  the  conduction  of  the  impulse  and 
the  operation  of  excitatory  and  inhibitory  synaptic 
junctions.    Recently,    numerous    electronmicroscopic 


59 


6o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Studies  (21,  22,  2;5,  48,  66,  67,  70)  have  revealed  it  as  a 
boundary  membrane  of  uniform  thickness,  about 
50  A,  which  stands  out  with  remarkable  clarity  from 
the  interior  of  the  neuron  and  its  surround.  There  is 
much  more  uncertainty  with  respect  to  the  chemical 
composition  of  the  membrane,  which  generally  is  sup- 
posed to  be  a  thin,  proi)ai)ly  bimolccular,  layer  of 
mixed  phospholipids  and  cholesterol,  supported  by  a 
protein  framework.  It  is  further  postulated  that  the 
transport  of  molecules  and  ions  across  this  membrane 
is  largely  a  diffusion  process,  the  respective  net  move- 
ments being  determined  by  the  electrochemical 
potentials  However,  metabolic  energy  must  also  be 
made  available  for  net  transport  against  the  electro- 
chemical gradients  of  such  ions  as  sodium  and  potas- 
sium. With  some  memljranes,  it  is  also  necessary  to 
postulate  that  specific  permeability  functions  are 
'built  in';  for  example,  in  all  membranes  giving  the 
self-regenerative  responses  that  are  characteristic  of 
impulses,  depolarization  initiates  a  brief  permeability 
to  sodium  ions;  and  at  excitatory  synapses  the  excita- 
tory transmitter  substance  probably  causes  the  mem- 
brane to  become  like  a  sieve  with  pores  permeable  to 
all  small  ions,  while  at  inhibitory  synapses  the  inhib- 
itory substance  causes  much  more  selective  ion 
permeability,  which  may,  however,  be  due  to  a  still 
finer  sieve-like  structure. 

It  will  emerge  in  the  sub.sequent  .sections  on  neuron 
physiology  that  as  yet  very  little  functional  significance 
can  be  attached  to  all  the  detailed  structural  features 
occurring  within  neurons,  which  are  well  described  in 
a  recent  review  by  Young  (79).  At  the  present  level  of 
understanding,  the  behavior  of  neurons  is  explained  in 
terms  of  the  properties  of  their  surface  membranes, 
including  the  specialized  surface  membranes  of  the 
synaptic  regions.  The  interior  is  assigned  a  function 
merely  on  account  of  its  ionic  composition  and  its 
specific  conductance.  Doubtless  this  unsatisfactory 
state  of  affairs  will  be  remedied  as  new  insights  are 
gained  into  the  metabolic  functions  of  the  nerve  cell 
and  their  integration  with  the  membrane  functions. 

Some  beginnings  have  already  been  made.  For 
example,  energy  derived  from  metabolic  processes  in 
the  neuron  is  necessary  in  order  to  move  ions  across 
the  surface  membrane  against  their  electrochemical 
potentials.  There  is  now  evidence  that,  with  the 
linked  transfer  of  sodium  outwards  and  potassium 
inwards,  the  rate  of  this  ionic  pump  is  determined  by 
the  internal  concentration  of  sodium  ions  (15,  16,  53, 
54).  Another  correlation  between  the  neuron  interior 
and  the  surface  membrane  is  beginning  to  emerge  in 
relation   to   the  synaptic  vesicles  in   the  presynaptic 


terminals.  There  is  evidence  .supporting  the  postulate- 
that  these  vesicles  are  concerned  in  the  quantal 
emission  of  transmitter  from  the  presynaptic  terminals- 
of  the  neuromuscular  junction  (26,  63,  70);  and  that 
the  level  of  the  membrane  potential  of  the  presynaptic 
terminals  determines  the  rate  of  emission  of  quanta 
therefrom,  the  rate  rising  by  more  than  a  million-fold 
during  a  nerve  impulse.  Thus  it  has  been  postulated 
that  in  some  way  the  properties  of  the  .surface  mem- 
brane are  able  to  influence  profoundly  the  state  of 
relatively  large  structures  (spheres  of  300  to  500  A  in 
diameter)  in  the  immediately  adjacent  cytoplasm 
(26,  63);  and,  by  analogy,  a  similar  postulate  has  been 
suggested  for  the  synaptic  vesicles  which  also  form 
characteristic  features  of  all  synaptic  junctions  that  on 
other  grounds  are  regarded  as  functioning  by  chemical 
transmission  (21,  29,  67). 

The  internal  structure  of  neurons  is  profoundly 
altered  in  pathological  states  induced,  for  example,  by 
section  of  the  axon  or  by  the  action  of  toxins  (4). 
There  is  good  evidence  that  such  a  striking  feature  as- 
the  Nissl  substance  or  ergastoplasm  is  concerned  in 
the  protein  manufacture  that  occurs  during  growth 
and  regeneration  (58).  But  as  yet  there  is  little  under- 
standing of  the  '  trophic'  action  which  the  cell  body 
exercises  on  the  axon,  apparently  by  maintaining  an 
intra-axonic  pressure  and  a  continual  tran.sfcr  of 
material  along  the  fiber  (79). 

Electronmicroscopy  has  already  contributed  much 
information  that  is  of  the  greatest  value  in  interpreting 
the  mode  of  operation  of  synapses.  Despite  the  very 
wide  range  in  the  grosser  features  of  synapses,  at  the 
electronmicroscopic  level  there  is  a  remarkable  simi- 
larity between  all  synapses  that  are  believed  to  work 
by  a  chemical  transmitter  mechanism  (fig.  i).  Es.sen- 
tially,  in  these  structures  considerable  areas  of  the 
presynaptic  and  postsynaptic  membranes  are  sepa- 
rated bv  a  very  narrow  cleft  that  shows  a  remarkable 
uniformity  in  width  for  any  one  type  of  synap.se  and 
that  varies  in  width  from  150  to  500  A  with  different 
types  Presumably,  this  accurate  apposition  of  the 
two  membranes  is  maintained  by  some  structural 
linkage  across  the  cleft,  which  appears  in  elcctron- 
microphotographs  as  a  granular  material  The  pre- 
synaptic and  postsynaptic  membranes  are  continuous 
with  the  surface  membranes  of  their  respecti\e  cells, 
neurons  or  effector  cells,  and  as  yet  ha\e  not  been 
shown  to  have  any  distinctive  structural  features 
except  the  deep  transverse  folds  that  distinguish  the 
subsynaptic  mu.scle  membrane  at  the  neuromuscular 
junction  (figs.  iZ),  E)  (19,  69,  70).  Finally,  in  all 
cheinical-transmitting  synapses  the  presynaptic  termi- 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION  6 1 


FIG.  I .  Drawings  showing  dimensions  and  form  of  various  types  of  synaptic  junctions  as  revealed 
by  electronmicroscopy.  In  all  transverse  sections  the  presynaptic  terminals  are  sho%vn  above  and  the 
postsynaptic  element  below.  In  addition  the  presynaptic  terminals  can  be  identified  by  the  contained 
synaptic  vesicles.  The  synaptic  cleft  is  seen  as  the  narrow  space  between  the  juxtaposed  presynaptic 
and  subsynaptic  membranes  and  is  shown  communicating  at  the  sides  of  the  synapse  with  the  inter- 
stitial spaces.  A,  A  large  synapse  on  a  motoneuron  of  the  abducens  nucleus.  [From  Palay  (67).]  B. 
Synapse  in  the  ventral  acoustic  ganglion  of  the  guinea  pig.  [From  de  Robertis  (21).]  C.  Synapse 
between  red  receptor  and  postsynaptic  cell  in  the  rabbit  retina.  [From  de  Robertis  &  Franchi  (23).] 
D,  E.  Elongated  nerve  terminal  of  amphibian  muscle  as  seen  from  above  (Z))  and  in  transverse 
section  (£).  The  naturally  occurring  irregularities  of  the  junctional  folds  are  neglected  in  order  to 
give  a  regular  geometrical  diagram  with  approximately  equivalent  dimensions.  A  junctional  fold  is 
shown  by  a  broken  line  in  E.  [From  data  and  figures  of  Couteaux  &  Taxi  (19)  and  Robertson 
(70).] 


nals  contain  the  characteristic  synaptic  vesicles  which 

o 

are  300  to  500  A  across  and  which  are  often  ckistered 
close  to  the  synaptic  region. 

The  word  synapse,  as  proposed  by  Sherrington 
(71),  may  be  applied  to  the  presynaptic  terminal  with 
its  contained  synaptic  vesicles,  the  synaptic  cleft  of 
150  to  500  A,  and  the  subsynaptic  membrane  with  its 
special  receptive  and  reactive  mechanism.  Later, 
when  the  mode  of  operation  of  synapses  is  discussed, 
it  will  appear  that  much  of  the  old  morphological 
characterization  of  synaptic  endings  is  of  little  signifi- 
cance, at  least  for  many  types  of  neurons.  Thus  the 
various  localizations  designated  axosomatic,  axoden- 
dritic and  axoaxonic  would  be  almost  equipotent  in 
their  action  except  for  those  neurons  that  have  very 
elongated  dendrites,  as  for  example  the  pyramidal 
cells  of  the  cortex.  Furthermore,  there  can  be  little 


significance  in  the  detailed  form  of  synapses  as  de- 
scribed bv  such  terms  as  hautons  lerminaux  and  en 
passant,  giant  club  endings,  basket-type  endings,  etc. 
[cf.  Bodian  (3)]. 


PHYSIOLOGICAL     PROPERTIES     OF    SURF.iiCE 
MEMBRANES  OF  NEURONS 

By  inserting  an  electrode  within  a  nerve  fiber  or 
the  soma  of  a  neuron  and  analyzing  the  potential 
changes  produced  by  current  pulses,  it  has  been 
shown  that  the  surface  membrane  has  a  high  electrical 
resistance,  corresponding  to  its  low  ionic  permeability, 
and  a  high  electrical  capacity,  as  would  be  expected 
for  a  membrane  no  more  than  50  A  thick.  The  elec- 
trical resistance  shows  wide  variations  with  different 


62 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


types  of  nerve  fibers  and  neurons,  the  values  ranging 
from  looo  to  approximately  io,ooon-cm-  for  squid 
and  sepia  giant  fibers,  respectively  (49,  77),  and  it 
probably  lies  within  the  range  of  500  to  loooli-cm^ 
for  mammalian  motoneurons  (15,  29,  44).  Values  for 
specific  capacitance  of  giant  fiber  membranes  range 
from  I  to  1.5  mF  per  cm-  and  for  mammalian  moto- 
neurons are  probably  at  least  3  nF  per  cm-.  In  addi- 
tion, there  is  a  considerable  potential  difference  across 
the  surface  membranes  of  neurons,  including  all 
their  branches,  the  inside  being  —50  to  —80  milli- 
volts relative  to  the  exterior  under  normal  resting 
conditions. 

It  may  be  claimed  that  only  one  hypothesis,  which 
may  be  termed  the  membrane  ionic  hypothesis, 
attempts  to  account  quantitatively  for  propagation 
within  neurons  both  of  impulses  and  of  the  events 
which  control  the  generation  of  impulses,  and  also  for 
transmission  across  synapses.  The  earliest  ionic 
hypothesis  was  proposed  by  Bernstein  (2)  in  1902. 
For  the  modern  version  of  this  ionic  hypothesis,  as 
applied  to  the  responses  within  a  neuron,  reference 
may  be  made  to  Hodgkin  (49,  50),  to  Hodgkin  & 
Huxley  (52)  and  to  Huxley  (57).  Its  application  to 
synaptic  transmission  has  been  specially  developed 
for  neuromuscular  junctions  and  the  synapses  on 
mammalian  motoneurons  (15,  16,  17,  26,  28,  29,  38, 

4i)- 

Essentially  it  is  postulated  that  the  resting  mem- 
brane potential  of  neurons  and  muscle  fibers  (  —  50  to 
—  100  mv)  is  due  to  the  relatively  free  diffusion  of  the 
small  ions,  K+  and  Cl~,  across  the  membrane,  while 
the  Na+  permeability  is  of  a  much  lower  order.  For 
example,  in  the  giant  axons  of  squid  the  resting  K+ 
and  Na+  conductances  are,  respectively,  about  0.5  and 
of  the  order  of  0.0 1  mmho  per  cm-.  As  a  consequence, 
an  electrical  potential  difference  is  set  up  across  the 
membrane  so  that  there  is  little  or  no  electrochemical 
potential  gradient  of  the  freely  diffusing  ions,  K+  and 
Cl~,  across  the  membrane  despite  the  very  large 
concentration  differences  that  obtain,  (Ki)/(Ko)  and 
(Clo)/CCli),  both  being  of  the  order  of  20  to  50.  It 
may  be  noted  that  subsidiary  hypotheses,  such  as  the 
ionic  pump  mentioned  in  the  preceding  section,  are 
required  in  order  to  explain  how  these  concentration 
differences  are  maintained  along  with  the  very  low 
internal  sodium  concentration.  It  is  further  postulated 
that,  if  the  resting  potential  of  the  membrane  is  sud- 
denly reduced  by  a  considerable  amount  (say  from  —  50 
mv  to  o),  both  the  Na+  and  K+  conductances  undergo 
characteristic  increases.  As  summarized  by  Huxley 
(57),  the  conductance  "  for  Na  ions  rises  in  one  or  two 


tenths  of  a  millisecond  to  perhaps  15  mmho,  cm-,  and 
then  falls  to  a  low  value  with  a  time  constant  of  about 
I  msec.  That  for  K  ions  does  not  change  noticeably  at 
first,  but  rises  along  an  S-shaped  curve,  becoming 
appreciable  as  the  Na  conductance  falls  from  its  peak, 
and  eventually  flattening  out  and  remaining  at  about 
20  mmho/cm-  as  long  as  the  membrane  potential 
difference  is  held  at  zero.  When  the  membrane  poten- 
tial difference  is  restored  to  its  ordinary  resting  value, 
the  K  conductance  returns  to  its  resting  value  along  an 
exponential  decay  curve,  without  an  S-shaped  start. 
The  Na  conductance  remains  low,  but  the  '  inactiva- 
tion'  which  caused  it  to  fall  after  its  peak  during  the 
period  at  zero  membrane  potential  difference  per- 
sists, decaying  exponentially  with  about  the  same 
time  constant  as  the  K  conductance."  Meanwhile  the 
Na  and  K  ions  have  been  moving  down  their  electro- 
chemical gradients.  For  a  giant  axon  there  is  a  gain 
in  Na  of  3  to  4  X  io~'-  moles  per  cm-  per  impulse  and 
a  loss  of  an  equivalent  amount  of  K. 

According  to  the  ionic  hypothesis,  the  membrane 
may  be  represented  by  an  electrical  diagram  (fig.  2) 
in  which  the  membrane  capacitance  (a)  is  shown  in 
parallel  with  two  battery-resistance  elements  (6  and 
(-)  representing,  respectively,  the  K  and  Na  difi'usion 
channels  across  the  membrane.  The  respective 
batteries  are  at  the  approximate  equilibrium  poten- 
tials for  K  and  Na  ions,  and  the  resistances  which 
represent  reciprocals  of  the  respective  conductances 
are  both  capable  of  variation  over  a  wide  range.  For 
the  .squid  axon  the  respective  resistances  of  the  resting 
membrane  are  about  2  X  lo'fi  cm^  and  lo^fi  cm-, 
while  during  activity  the  values  are  as  low  as  25^  cm^ 
and  loi]  cm-. 

On  the  basis  of  quantitative  studies  of  the  time 
courses  of  the  conductance  changes  as  produced  by 
a  wide  range  of  membrane  potential  changes,  it  has 
been  possible  (52)  to  set  up  differential  equations 
which  relate  three  parameters  to  the  membrane  po- 
tential changes,  viz.  the  'turning  on'  of  the  Na  con- 
ductance, the  'turning  on'  of  the  K  conductance  and 
the  'inactivation'  of  the  Na  conductance,  and  in  which 
all  the  coefficients  are  experimentally  determined. 
These  equations  give  a  very  satisfactory  quantitative 
account  of  a  wide  range  of  performance  of  the  giant 
fibers  from  which  the  coefficients  were  derived.  It  will 
suffice  to  show  how  the  propagation  of  the  nerve 
impulse  is  explained. 

The  explanation  of  the  propagation  of  the  nerve 
impulse  is  based  on  measurements  of  the  cable 
properties  of  the  nerve  fiber  in  addition  to  the  differ- 
ential equation  relating  the  ionic  conductances  to  the 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION  63 


Vnb 


•  External  fluid 


'\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/\/  Inlenor  of  fibre 


llSmV 


12  mV 


FIG.  2.  Theoretical  action  potential  (F)  and  membrane  conductance  changes  gNa  and  ^k  obtained 
by  solving  the  equations  derived  by  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (52)  for  the  giant  axon  at  i8.5°C.  Inset 
shows  diagram  of  an  element  of  the  excitable  membrane  of  a  nerve  fiber — a,  constant  capacity; 
b,  channel  for  K+;  c,  channel  for  Na+.  [From  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (52);  Huxley  (57)-] 


membrane  potential.  At  any  instant  the  nerve  im- 
pulse will  be  extended  as  a  potential  change  along 
the  nerve  fiber  as  shown  in  figure  35.  According  to 
the  ionic  hypothesis,  there  will  be  a  net  inward  move- 
ment of  Na  ions  during  the  rising  phase  of  the  impulse 
(figs.  2,  3.4)  because  the  Na  conductance  has  been 
greatly  increased  by  the  depolarization  so  that  Na 
ions  move  freely  down  their  electrochemical  gradient 
carrying  positive  charges  inwards,  thus  adding  to  the 
depolarization  and  hence  to  the  Na  conductance.  In 
this  self-regenerative  manner,  when  the  level  of 
depolarization  of  any  element  of  the  nerve  membrane 
increases  above  a  critical  value,  it  causes  the  mem- 
brane potential  to  be  carried  almost  up  to  the  Na 
equilibrium  potential  which  is  about  +50  mv,  i.e. 
internally  positive  (fig.  2).  The  delayed  development 
of  the  other  two  ionic  processes  checks  this  potential 
change  and  eventually  restores  the  resting  membrane 
potential;  the  Na  conductance  is  inactivated  and  the 
K  conductance  increases  so  that,  during  the  falling 
phase  of  the  impulse,  the  membrane  potential  is 
dominated  by  the  flux  of  K  ions  moving  outwards 
along     their    electrochemical    gradient    across     the 


membrane  (figs.  2,  3.-1},  which  eventually  is  restored 
to  its  original  resting  potential  close  to  the  potassium 
equilibrium  potential.  Propagation  occurs  because 
of  the  cable  properties  of  the  nerve  fiber,  current 
flowing  outwards  across  the  membrane  ahead  of  the 
impulse  in  the  circuits,  as  shown  diagrammatically  in 
figure  3C.  This  current  efTects  a  discharge  of  the  mem- 
brane capacitance  so  that  in  the  zone  ahead  of  the 
impulse  the  membrane  is  depolarized  sufficiently  to 
initiate  the  regenerative  increase  in  Na  conductance, 
by  which  time  the  impulse  may  be  said  to  have 
arrived  at  this  new  zone,  which  will  in  turn  go  through 
the  conductance  changes  outlined  above.  It  will  be 
appreciated  that  propagation  will  be  a  continuous 
and  uniform  process  along  a  stretch  of  nerve  with 
uniform  properties.  The  propagation  velocity  calcu- 
lated from  the  differential  equation  and  the  measured 
cable  properties  of  a  nerve  fiber  is  not  only  of  the 
correct  order,  but  is  in  very  close  agreement  with  that 
actually  observed  (52).  Saltatory  propagation  along 
the  nodal  structure  of  a  medullated  nerve  also  can  be 
satisfactorily  explained  by  the  occurrence  of  essen- 
tially similar  processes  at  each  node.  This  propagation 


64 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


A    ^15? 


Fig.  3.  A.  Diagram  showing  postulated  movement  of  sodium 
and  potassium  ions  across  the  membrane  during  an  impulse 
advancing  in  the  direction  of  arrow,  and  the  resulting  alteration 
of  charge  on  the  membrane  and  its  recovery.  B.  Potential 
distribution  of  the  impulse  along  a  nerve  or  muscle  fiber. 
C.  Resulting  flow  of  electric  current  both  in  the  external 
medium  and  within  the  fiber.  Note  the  reversal  of  membrane 
potential  during  the  spike.  Figure  3/J  is  drawn  so  that  the 
impulse  is  at  approximately  the  same  position  as  in  figure  3.4 
and  C. 


is    treated    very    iulK    in    the   following   chapter    by 
Tasaki. 

After  the  events  depicted  in  figures  2  and  3,  the 
ionic  hypothesis  would  predict  that  a  length  of  nerve 
fiber  would  have  gained  a  quantity  of  Na  ions  that 
was  at  least  adequate  to  displace  the  charge  on  its 
capacitance  so  that  there  is  the  maximum  change  in 
the  membrane  potential,  and  that  there  would  also 
have  been  an  equivalent  loss  of  K  ions  in  the  recharg- 
ing process.  The  actually  observed  values  have  been 
several  times  larger,  which  is  to  be  expected  because 
the  periods  of  Na  entry  and  K  emission  overlap  so  that 
much  of  the  ionic  influx  cancels  out  as  far  as  the 
membrane  potential  is  concerned.  Thus  the  immediate 
energy  source  for  the  propagation  of  the  impulse 
derives  from  the  concentration  batteries  for  Na  and  K 
ions,  and  metabolic  energy  is  only  later  required  in 
order  to  restore  the  ionic  composition.  However,  the 
ionic  flux  per  impulse  is  so  small  relati\e  to  the  ionic 
composition  of  the  fiber  that,  even  in  the  alj.sence  of  a 
restorative  process,  many  thousands  of  impulses  can 
be    propagated    along    large    nerve    fibers    without 


significaniK  changing  the  effectiveness  of  the  con- 
centration batteries. 

The  ionic  hypothesis  can  also  explain  satisfactorily 
a  great  many  other  properties  of  nerve  fibers  [(cf. 
Hodgkin  (49);  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  C52)],  for  example 
the  subthreshold  and  threshold  phenomena  including 
the  all-or-nothing  behavior,  the  refractory  period  fol- 
lowing the  impulse,  the  effects  of  anelectrotonus  and 
catelectrotonus,  including  accommodation,  the  effects 
produced  on  the  nerve  impulse  and  the  other  re- 
sponses by  changing  the  Na  or  K  concentrations,  or 
both,  in  the  external  medium  and  in  the  axoplasm 
(54).  This  is  such  an  immensely  impressive  per- 
formance that  the  ionic  hypothesis  of  the  nerve  fiber 
must  rank  as  one  of  the  great  conceptual  achieve- 
ments in  biology. 

It  is  admitted  that  as  yet  the  ionic  hypothesis,  in  so 
far  as  it  has  been  formulated,  does  not  give  a  com- 
plete description  of  the  behavior  of  the  nerve  mem- 
brane. For  example  the  nature  of  the  specific  changes 
in  Na  and  K  conductance  is  not  explained;  the  in- 
tensity-time courses  of  changes  are  merely  measured 
and  utilized  in  the  explanations.  The  effect  of  external 
calcium  ions  on  these  conductances  also  is  not  yet 
understood.  Again,  nothing  is  known  about  the  manner 
in  which  metabolic  energy  is  employed  to  drive 
sodium  and  potassium  ions  across  the  membrane 
against  their  electrochemical  gradient. 

As  would  be  expected,  such  a  comprehensive  and 
precisely  formulated  hypothesis  has  been  subjected  to 
much  critical  attack.  However  much  of  this  criticism 
has  been  based  on  imperfectly  controlled  experiments. 
For  example  deviations  from  the  predicted  effects  of 
variations  in  the  external  potassium  concentrations 
on  the  resting  membrane  potential  probably  have 
been  largely  due  to  secondary  changes  in  the  internal 
potassium.  In  this  context  great  significance  attaches 
to  the  recent  experiments  of  Hodgkin  &  Horowicz 
(51)  on  the  membrane  potential  of  isolated  single 
muscle  fibers.  Extracellular  diffusion  time  is  thus  re- 
duced to  a  minimum,  so  that  a  steady  membrane 
potential  is  observed  within  a  second  of  changing  the 
external  ionic  composition  and  thu«  before  there  is  any 
appreciable  change  in  the  internal  composition. 
Under  such  conditions,  with  changes  in  (Kq),  the  ob- 
served membrane  potentials  agree  very  closely  with 
those  predicted  by  the  ionic  hypothesis.  It  was  also 
remarkable  that,  making  use  of  the  anomalous 
rectification  in  K  ionic  diffusion  across  the  membrane 
[cf.  Katz  (59)],  it  was  possible  by  changing  the  internal 
composition  of  the  muscle  fiber  to  have  a  membrane 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION 


65 


the  potential  of  which  was  virtually  controlled  by  the 
(C'i„)/(Cli)  ratio  and  then  later  to  restore  the  normal 
ionic  composition  of  the  fiber,  as  revealed  by  a  normal 
behavior  of  the  membrane  potential  to  variations  in 
(K„). 

In  conclusion  it  may  he  stated  that,  though  detailed 
modifications  and  developments  of  the  ionic  hypoth- 
esis are  recjuired  in  order  to  explain  such  phenomena 
as  the  falling  phases  of  the  action  potentials  of  medul- 
lated  nerve  fibers  and  cardiac  muscle  fibers  and  the 
effect  thereon  of  repolarizing  currents,  in  essentials 
the  ionic  membrane  hypothesis  has  survived  the  most 
severe  tests  and  remains  as  the  only  conceptual  frame- 
work for  our  discussion  of  the  electrical  events  that 
are  so  essentially  concerned  in  all  activities  of  the 
neuron.  It  will  therefore  be  pertinent  to  consider  now 
the  mode  of  operation  of  synapses  in  the  light  of  the 
ionic  hypothesis. 


TRANSMISSION  BETWEEN  NEURONS 

The  synapse  is  a  device  for  the  transmission  of 
information  from  one  neuron  to  another.  Excitatory 
synaptic  action  is  effective  only  in  so  far  as  it  leads  to 
the  discharge  of  an  impulse  by  the  postsynaptic 
neuron,  for  only  under  such  conditions  does  this 
neuron  in  turn  exert  effective  action  on  other  neurons. 
It  may  be  provisionally  concluded  from  the  available 
experimental  evidence  that  any  neuron,  other  than  a 
primary  sensory  neuron,  requires  excitatory  synaptic 
action  in  order  to  generate  an  impulse.  In  the  absence 
of  an  afferent  input  even  the  most  complex  assem- 
blages of  neurons  remain  silent,  as  may  be  seen  in  the 
isolated  cortical  slabs  of  Burns  (10). 

On  the  other  hand,  inhibitory  synaptic  action 
attempts  to  suppress  the  discharge  of  impulses  and  is 
effective  in  so  far  as  it  diminishes  or  shortens  the  dis- 
charge produced  by  any  given  synaptic  excitation. 
Inhibition  can  be  thought  of  as  exercising  a  sculptur- 
ing role  on  what  would  otherwise  be  the  massive 
incoordinate  activity  of  a  convulsing  nervous  system, 
thus  reducing  it  to  the  organized  responses  character- 
istic of  normal  nervous  activity.  However,  just  as 
with  the  excitatory  synapses,  inhibitory  synap.ses 
require  activation  by  presynaptic  impulses.  Hence,  an 
investigation  of  the  transmis.sion  between  neurons 
can  be  reduced  to  a  study  of  the  mode  of  operation  of 
excitatory  and  inhibitory  synapses.  It  will  emerge 
that    the   ionic    hypothesis   of  the    nerve   membrane 


provides  the  basis  for  our  atteinpls  to  understand  both 
these  types  of  synaptic  activity. 

Excitatory  Synaptic  Action 

Excitatory  synaptic  action  on  neurons  is  exhibited 
in  its  simplest  form  by  the  monosynaptic  action  which 
afferent  impulses  from  the  annulospiral  endings  of 
muscle  spindles  exert  on  motoneurons.  When  recorded 
by  an  intracellular  electrode,  the  monosynaptic 
action  by  a  single  volley  generates  a  depolarizing 
potential,  the  excitatory  postsynaptic  potential 
(EPSP),  that  runs  virtually  the  same  time  course 
regardless  of  volley  size  (fig.  4.4  to  C).  This  observa- 


FiG.  4.  A  to  C.  EPSP's  obtained  in  a  biceps-semitendinosus 
motoneuron  with  afferent  voIley.s  of  different  size.  Inset  records 
at  the  left  of  the  main  records  show  the  afferent  \olley  recorded 
near  the  entry  of  the  dorsal  nerve  roots  into  the  spinal  cord. 
They  are  taken  with  negativity  downward  and  at  a  constant 
amplification  for  which  no  scale  is  given.  Records  of  EPSP  are 
taken  at  an  amplification  that  decreases  in  steps  from  A  lo  C  as 
the  response  increases.  Separate  vertical  scales  are  given  for 
each  record  of  EPSP.  All  records  are  formed  by  superposition  of 
about  40  faint  traces.  D  to  G.  Intracellularly-recorded  po- 
tentials of  a  gastrocnemius  motoneuron  (resting  membrane 
potential,  —70  mv)  evoked  by  monosynaptic  activation  that 
was  progiessively  increased  from  D  to  G.  The  lower  traces  are 
the  electrically  differentiated  records,  the  double-headed  arrows 
indicating  the  onsets  of  the  IS  spikes  in  E  to  G.  HtoK.  Intra- 
cellular records  evoked  by  monosynaptic  activation  that  was 
applied  at  12.0  msec,  after  the  onset  of  a  depolarizing  pulse 
whose  strength  is  indicated  in  m^ia.  A  pulse  of  20  m^ua  was  just 
below  threshold  for  generating  a  spike.  H  shows  control  EPSP 
in  the  absence  of  a  depolarizing  pulse.  Lower  traces  give 
electrically  differentiated  records.  Note  that  the  spikes  are 
truncated.  [From  Coombs  el  at.  (14).] 


66 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tion  indicates  that  each  excitatory  presynaptic  impulse 
generates  in  the  postsynaptic  neuron  a  potential 
change  of  this  same  time  course,  and  that  the  recorded 
EPSP's  of  figure  4.4  to  C  are  produced  by  a  simple 
summation  of  these  elemental  EPSP's.  It  thus  pro- 
vides an  illustration  of  the  classical  concept  of  spatial 
summation  (72,  73). 

As  shown  in  figure  4D  to  G,  if  the  EPSP  is  increased 
beyond  a  critical  threshold  level,  it  causes  the  neuron 
to  discharge  an  impulse,  the  latency  being  briefer  the 
larger  the  EPSP.  In  figure  4.E,  F,  G  the  increase  of  the 
EPSP  to  above  threshold  was  brought  about  by  in- 
creasing the  size  of  the  presynaptic  volley,  but,  as 
would  be  expected,  the  EPSP  can  also  be  made  to 
generate  an  impulse  by  conditioning  procedures  that 
change  the  membrane  potential  towards  the  critical 
threshold  level.  For  example  in  figure  4/  to  A'  the 
same  EPSP  as  in  figure  4//  was  made  effective  by  the 
operation  of  a  background  depolarizing  current 
which  was  commenced  1 2  msec,  before  and  which 
changed  the  membrane  potential  by  the  amount 
shown  in  each  record.  The  impulse  is  seen  to  arise 
(at  the  arrows)  at  a  total  level  of  depolarization  of 
about  18  mv,  which  is  made  up  in  varying  proportions 
by   the  conditioning  depolarization   and   the   super- 


imposed EPSP.  The  threshold  level  of  depolarization 
may  be  attained  also  by  superimposing  an  EPSP  on 
the  depolarization  produced  by  a  preceding  EPSP 
(temporal  summation),  as  is  illustrated  by  Grundfest 
(Chapter  \',  fig.  i  7). 

All  these  investigations  conform  with  the  hypothesis 
that  synaptic  excitatory  action  is  effective  in  generat- 
ing an  impulse  solely  by  the  depolarization  of  the 
neuron,  i.e.  by  producing  the  EPSP  (17,  28,  29,  44). 
As  far  as  the  generation  of  an  impulse  by  the  EPSP  is 
concerned,  the  same  processes  obtain  as  with  the 
propagation  of  an  impulse  from  one  part  of  a  neuron 
to  another. 

In  order  to  produce  the  EPSP,  the  activated  syn- 
apses must  cause  an  electric  current  to  be  generated 
which  depolarizes  the  postsynaptic  membrane.  Thus, 
as  shown  in  figure  ^B,  a  current  must  flow  inwards 
immediately  under  the  activated  synapses,  i.e.  across 
the  subsynaptic  membrane,  in  order  that  a  return 
current  may  flow  outward  across  the  remainder  of  the 
postsynaptic  membrane,  so  depolarizing  it.  When  a 
brief  current  pulse  is  applied  across  the  membrane,  it 
builds  up  a  potential  difference  that  on  cessation  of 
the  current  decays  considerably  faster  than  the  EPSP 
(12).  Hence  it  is  postulated  that  the  current  producing 


V/scc 


o 


NORMAL     MEMBRANE 


3x|6'f 


6 


E     SYNAPSES 


as  low  OS 


5x  10  n 


FIG.  5.  A.  The  continuous  line  is  the  mean  of  several  monosynaptic  EPSP's,  while  the  broken  line 
shows  the  time  course  of  the  subsynaptic  current  required  to  generate  this  potential  change.  B. 
Diagram  showing  an  activated  excitatory  knob  and  the  postsynaptic  membrane.  .As  indicated  by  the 
scales  for  distance,  the  synaptic  cleft  is  shown  at  10  times  the  scale  for  width  as  against  length.  The 
current  generating  the  EPSP  passes  in  through  the  cleft  and  inward  across  the  activated  subsynaptic 
membrane.  [From  Coombs  et  al.  (12).]  C.  Formal  electrical  diagram  of  the  membrane  of  a  motoneu- 
ron with,  on  the  right  side,  the  circuit  through  the  subsynaptic  areas  of  the  membrane  that  are 
activated  in  producing  the  monosynaptic  EPSP.  Maximum  activation  of  these  areas  would  be 
indicated  symbolically  by  closing  the  switch. 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION 


67 


the  EPSP  is  not  suddenly  switched  off  after  the  summit 
of  the  EPSP,  but  that,  as  shown  in  the  analysis  of 
figure  5/I  (broken  line),  a  small  residual  current 
continues  to  flow  and  thus  delays  the  repolarization 
during  the  decline  of  the  EPSP  (continuous  line).  It 
will  be  appreciated  that  the  EPSP's  of  figures  4  and 
^A  are  produced  by  the  operation  on  the  neuron  of 
the  postsynaptic  currents  generated  by  many  synaptic 
knobs  that  have  been  activated  simultaneously  by 
the  afferent  volley. 

By  passing  an  extrinsic  current  across  the  neuronal 
membrane  it  has  been  possible  even  to  reverse  the 
potential  across  it,  its  interior  then  being  po.sitive  to 
the  exterior.  When  this  occurs,  the  EP.SP  is  also 
reversed  in  sign  (cf.  Grundfest,  Chapter  V,  fig.  35), 
which  indicates  a  reversal  of  the  postsynaptic  currents 
shown  in  figure  5^  and  of  the  ionic  flux  across  the 
subsynaptic  membrane  (17).  The  effects  on  the  EPSP 
of  diminution  and  reversal  of  the  membrane  potential 
and  of  changes  in  the  ionic  composition  of  the  neuron 
are  explicable  by  the  postulate  that  the  activated  sub- 
synaptic membrane  becomes  permeable  to  all  small 
ions,  such  as  Na"*",  K"*"  and  Cl~.  The  time  course  of 
this  permeability  change  is  given  by  the  broken  line 
of  figure  ^A,  and  its  effect  on  the  membrane  potential 
can  be  derived  from  the  electrical  diagrain  of  figure 
5C.  A  similar  investigation  on  the  endplate  potential 
of  the  neuromuscular  junction  (24,  26;  Fatt,  Chapter 
VI)  has  shown  that  reversal  occurs  at  a  membrane 
potential  of  about  —  1 5  mv,  which  would  be  close  to 
the  liquid-junction  potential  between  the  muscle  fiber 
and  its  environment.  More  accurate  investigations  on 
the  EPSP  may  likewise  reveal  that  a  battery  of  about 
—  1 5  mv  should  be  inserted  in  the  synaptic  component 
of  the  diagram  in  figure  5C. 

It  can  now  be  taken  as  established  that  transmission 
across  synapses  occurs  not  by  the  spread  of  electrical 
currents,  but  by  the  specific  chemical  substances 
which  impulses  cause  to  be  liberated  from  the  pre- 
synaptic membranes  (29,  38,  43).  These  substances 
alter  the  ionic  permeability  of  the  subsynaptic  mem- 
brane and  consequently  initiate  specific  ionic  fluxes 
across  this  membrane.  These  fluxes  in  turn  are  re- 
sponsible for  the  postsynaptic  currents  that  cause  the 
transient  depolarizations  or  hyperpolarizations  of  the 
postsynaptic  membrane  which  are  produced  respec- 
tively by  excitatory  or  inhibitory  action  (16,  1 7).  Since 
it  gives  the  time  course  of  the  ionic  permeability 
change,  the  broken  line  of  figure  5.-I  may  be  taken  to 
give  the  time  cour.se  of  action  on  the  subsynaptic 
membrane  of  the  brief  jet  of  excitatory  transmitter 
substance   that  a  presynaptic   impulse  causes   to  be 


emitted  from  the  presynaptic  knob.  .Acetylcholine  is 
the  transmitter  substance  at  a  few  types  of  central 
synapse,  but  the  excitatory  transmitter  has  not  yet 
been  identified  for  the  great  majority. 

Impulses  can  also  be  generated  in  a  nerve  cell  by 
another  method  that  is  of  particular  value  in  relation 
to  the  problem  of  locating  the  site  at  which  impulses 
arise  in  nerve  cells.  When  the  a.xon  of  a  nerve  cell  is 
stimulated,  an  impulse  travels  antidromically  up  to  the 
nerve  cell  and  usually  invades  it,  generating  an  anti- 
dromic spike  potential  as  in  figure  6A.  When  thus 
recorded  by  a  microelectrode  in  the  soma,  the  anti- 
dromic spike  potential  has  two  main  components,  as 
shown  by  the  step  on  the  rising  phase  which  is  greatly 
accentuated  in  the  electrically  differentiated  record 
lying  immediately  below  the  potential  record  in 
figure  6.4.  Evidence  from  recent  intensive  investiga- 
tions (i,  7,  13,  39,  40,  46)  can  all  be  satisfactorily 
explained  by  the  postulate  that  the  initial  small  spike 
is  generated  by  the  impulse  in  the  initial  segment  of 
the  neuron  (axon  hillock  plus  nonmeduUated  axon), 
while  the  later  large  spike  is  produced  when  the 
impulse  invades  the  soma-dendritic  membrane  (13, 
46).  The  two  spikes  may  therefore  be  called  the  IS 
and  SD  spikes. 

When  the  neuronal  spike  potentials  generated  by 
synaptic  or  direct  stimulation  are  recorded  at  suffi- 
cient speed,  they  are  likewise  seen  to  be  compounded 
of  IS  and  SD  spikes,  particularly  in  the  differentiated 
records  (fig.  6B),  though  the  separation  is  always  less 
evident  than  with  the  corresponding  antidromic 
spike  potential.  It  must  therefore  be  postulated  that 
the  EPSP  produced  by  the  activation  of  synapses 
covering  the  soma  and  dendrites  is  effective  not  by 
generating  an  impulse  in  these  regions,  but  by  the 
electrotonic  spread  of  the  depolarization  to  the  initial 
segment,  as  is  illustrated  by  the  lines  of  current  flow 
in  figure  6C.  By  recording  the  impulse  discharged 
along  the  motor  nerve  fiber  in  the  ventral  root  it  is 
found  that  usually  this  impulse  started  to  propagate 
down  the  meduUated  axon  about  0.05  msec,  after  the 
initiation  of  the  IS  spike,  i.e.  the  meduUated  axon  is 
usually  excited  secondarily  to  the  initial  segment  (14). 
The  critical  level  of  depolarization  for  generating  an 
impulse  thus  gives  the  threshold  for  the  IS  mem- 
brane, as  marked  by  the  horizontal  arrow  labelled 
IS  in  figure  65,  and  not  of  the  SD  membrane.  An 
approximate  measure  of  the  threshold  for  the  SD 
membrane  is  given  by  the  membrane  potential  ob- 
tained at  the  first  sign  of  inflection  produced  by  the 
incipient  SD  spike,  as  is  indicated  by  the  differentiated 
records  in  figure  6.4  and  B.  This  potential  is  measured 


68  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 

r' 


100     ^ 

ImV 


FIG.  6.  Tracings  of  intracellularly  recorded  spike  potentials  evoked  by  antidromic  (.4)  and  mono- 
synaptic (B)  stimulation  of  a  motoneuron,  respectively.  [From  Coombs  et  al.  (14).]  The  lower  traces 
shosv  the  electrically  differentiated  records.  Perpendicular  lines  are  drawn  from  the  origins  of  the 
IS  and  SD  spikes,  as  indicated  in  the  differentiated  records,  the  respective  threshold  depolarizations 
being  thus  determined  from  the  potential  records  and  indicated  by  horizontal  lines  labelled  respec- 
tively IS  and  SD.  C.  Diagram  showing  the  lines  of  current  flow  that  occur  when  a  synaptically 
induced  depolarization  of  the  soma-dendritic  membrane  electrotonically  spreads  to  the  initiaj 
segment. 


at  the  levels  of  the  horizontal  SD  arrow.s  and  is 
approximately  the  same  for  the  antidromically  and 
synaptically  evoked  spikes,  as  illustrated  in  figure  'oA 
and  B.  Synaptic  excitatory  action  thus  generates  an 
SD  spike  not  directly  by  its  depolarizing  action,  but 
only  indircctK  through  the  mediation  of  the  IS  spike 
which  lifts  the  depolarization  of  the  SD  membrane  to 
threshold  by  currents  that  flow  in  the  reverse  direction 
to  those  drawn  in  figure  6C'. 

With  normal  motoneurons  the  threshold  level  of 
depolarization  has  always  been,  as  in  figure  6.4  and  B, 
much  higher  for  the  SD  membrane  than  for  the  IS 
membrane.  There  has  been  a  consideraljle  range  in 
the  threshold  values  for  motoneurons  that  are  shown 
by  their  resting  and  spike  potentials  to  be  in  good  con- 
dition. The  IS  threshold  has  ranged  from  6  to  18  mv, 
and  the  SD  threshold  from  20  to  37  mv  (14).  However, 
for  any  one  motoneuron  the  SD  threshold  has  been 
about  two  to  three  times  the  IS  threshold.  Several 
other  types  of  neurons  in  the  central  nervous  system 
also  reveal  a  threshold  difference  between  the  IS  and 
SD  membranes.  The  functional  significance  of  these 
distinctive  threshold  areas  of  neurons  will  be  con- 
sidered after  synaptic  inhibitory  action  has  been 
considered. 

The  difference  in  threshold  between  the  IS  and  SD 
membranes  must  not  be  confused  with  the  concept 


that  membranes  excited  by  chemical  transmitter  are 
inexcitable  electrically  (cf.  Grundfest,  Chapter  V). 
This  concept  would  be  applicable  merely  to  the  sub- 
synaptic  areas  of  the  SD  ineitibrane  and  not  to  the 
whole  of  that  membrane.  It  should  be  noted  that  the 
receptor  membrane  of  the  bare  nerve  ending  in  the 
Pacinian  corpuscle  also  appears  to  be  inexcitable 
electrically,  though  acting  as  a  primary  focus  for 
depolarizing  the  first  node  of  the  meduUated  axon 
(27;  Gray,  Chapter  IV).  There  is  some  analogy  here 
with  the  SD  membrane  acting  to  depolarize  the  IS 
membrane,  so  generating  an  impulse  there;  but  the 
analogy  does  not  hold  for  subsequent  e\ents  because 
the  impulse  in  the  IS  membrane  usually  invades  the 
SD  membrane,  whereas  with  the  Pacinian  corpu.scle 
there  is  no  such  antidromic  invasion. 

Inhibitory  Synaptic  Action 

Strictlv,  the  concept  of  inhibition  is  restricted  to 
depressions  of  neuronal  excitability  which  occur 
independently  of  any  conditioning  excitatory  synaptic 
activity  on  that  neuron,  and  also  independently  of  any 
depression  of  the  excitatory  .synaptic  bombardment 
that  is  employed  in  testing  for  the  suspected  inhibition. 
It  mav  be  noted  that  conditioning  by  large  afferent 
voUevs  causes  a  fairlv  prolonged  depression  in  the  size 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY — INTRODUCTION 


69 


of  the  primary  afferent  volley  and  hence  depresses  its 
excitatory  action  (8,  45,  55).  This  effect  has  been 
attributed  to  the  dorsal  root  reflex  and  the  dorsal  root 
potential  set  up  by  the  powerful  conditioning  volley 
(8)  and  probably  is  of  little  significance  with  more 
physiological  types  of  afferent  input.  Apart  from  this 
effect  it  has  been  shown  that  inhibitory  actions  on 
motoneurons  are  explained  satisfactorily  by  the  tran- 
sient increase  which  is  produced  in  their  membrane 
potentials  and  which  has  been  designated  the  inhibi- 
tory postsynaptic  potential,  IPSP  (6,  16,  18).  A  com- 
parable synaptic  inhibitory  action  has  been  observed 
with  crustacean  stretch  receptor  cells  (60),  and  has 
also  been  recorded  on  the  neurons  of  Clarke's  column 
by   Curtis,   Eccles   &    Lundberg  (19a). 

As  shown  in  figure  75  to  H,  a  single  volley  in  the 
afferent  fibers  from  annulospiral  endings  in  quadriceps 
muscle  evokes  a  hyperpolarizing  response,  the  inhibi- 
tory postsynaptic  potential  (IPSP)  in  a  motoneuron 
of  the  antagonist  muscle  (biceps-semitendinosus).  The 
IPSP  is  observed  to  be  increased  in  a  series  of  stages 
as  the  afferent  volley  is  increased  in  size,  but  it  is  not 
altered  in  time  course,  showing  that  a  simple  spatial 
summation  occurs  when  several  inhibitory  synapses  on 
the  same  neuron  are  simultaneously  activated.  With 
the  maximum  spatial  summation  in  figure  jE  the 
membrane  potential  was  increased  from  —60  to 
-63.5  mv. 


In  order  to  produce  the  observed  hyperpolariza- 
tion,  current  must  be  flowing  inward  across  the  moto- 
neuronal  membrane  in  general,  and  there  must  be  a 
corresponding  outward  current  in  the  region  of  the 
activated  inhibitory  synapses  (fig.  8A,  inset).  As  with 
the  excitatory  synaptic  action  in  figure  5^,  the  time 
course  of  the  current  that  produces  the  IPSP  may  be 
determined  if  the  time  constant  of  the  membrane  is 
known.  The  broken  line  in  figure  8.4  plots  the  time 
course  so  determined  and  shows  that  the  high  intensity 
phase  has  virtually  the  same  time  course  as  with 
excitatory  synaptic  action,  though  there  is  much  less 


r-r-i~r-rT-n 

msec 


Fig.  7.  A  to  H.  Lower  records  give  intracellular  responses  of 
a  biceps-semitendinosus  motoneuron  to  a  quadriceps  volley  of 
progressively  increasing  size,  as  is  shown  by  the  upper  records 
which  are  recorded  from  the  si.xth  lumbar  dorsal  root  by  a 
surface  electrode  (downward  deflections  indicating  negativity). 
All  records  are  formed  by  tfie  superposition  of  about  40  faint 
traces. 


B  I   ELEMENT        ^  ORDINARY 


i-l 


ELEMENT 


I  70  mV  I  90  mV  |  TO  mV 

I T     .    T 


INSIDE        CELL 


FIG.  8.  A.  Continuous  line  plots  the  mean  time  course  of  the  IPSP  set  up  in  a  biceps-semitendinosus 
motoneuron  by  a  single  quadriceps  la  volley.  The  measured  time  constant  for  the  membrane  was 
2.8  msec.  The  broken  line  gives  the  time  course  of  the  inhibitory  subsynaptic  current  that  would 
produce  the  IP.SP,  the  calculation  being  similar  to  that  used  in  deriving  figure  ^A.  Inset  shows  lines 
of  postsynaptic  current  flow  in  relationship  to  an  inhibitory  synaptic  knob.  B.  Diagrammatic 
representation  of  the  electrical  properties  of  an  ordinary  element  on  the  neuronal  membrane  and  of 
an  inhibitory  element  with  K+  and  Cl~  ion  components  in  parallel.  Further  description  in  the 
text. 


70 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


residual  action.  By  investigating  the  effects  of  varying 
the  membrane  potential  by  current  applied  through 
the  microelectrode  (cf  Grundfest,  Chapter  V,  fig.  1 2), 
it  has  been  shown  that  the  IPSP  is  produced  by  a 
process  of  ionic  diffusion  across  the  subsynaptic 
membrane  that  has  an  equilibrium  potential  at  about 
10  mv  more  hyperpolarized  than  the  resting  mem- 
brane potential,  i.e.  at  about  —80  mv  (16).  Further- 
more, it  has  been  shown  by  ionophoretic  injection 
through  the  microelectrode  that  this  ionic  diflusion  is 
satisfactorily  explained  by  the  hypothesis  that  the 
inhiijitory  synaptic  transmitter  increases  the  perme- 
ability of  the  subsynaptic  membrane  to  ions  below  a 
critical  size,  e.g.  to  K+  and  Cl~,  and  not  to  somewhat 
larger  ions,  e.g.  to  Na+  (16;  Grundfest,  Chapter  V, 
fig.  12).  This  type  of  ionic  mechanism  appears  to 
occur  with  all  types  of  central  inhibition  so  far  investi- 
gated and  also  with  the  IPSP  of  the  crustacean 
stretch  receptor  cells  (37,  60).  It  is  remarkaljle  that 
a  somewhat  similar  ionic  mechanism  explains  the 
vagal  inhibitory  action  on  the  heart  (25,  56,  76)  and 
probably  for  the  inhibitory  action  on  crustacean 
muscle  (42). 

The  electrical  diagram  in  figure  8C  illustrates  the 
hypothesis  that  the  inhibitory  transmitter  increases 
the  conductance  of  the  subsynaptic  membrane  to 
both  K+  and  Cl~  ions,  which  have  the  equilibrium 
potentials  indicated  by  the  respective  batteries,  and 
so  cau.ses  the  flow  of  a  current  (fig.  Bfi)  which  tends  to 
hyperpolarize  the  rest  of  the  neuronal  membrane  to 
about  —80  mv,  which  is  the  mean  of  the  equilibrium 
potentials  for  K+  and  Cl~  ions. 

Factors  Controlling  Impulse  Generation 

The  currents  which  flow  from  the  subsynaptic 
membrane  to  exert  a  hyperpolarizing  action  on  the 
motoneuronal  membrane  and  .set  up  an  IPSP  (fig.  8 A, 
inset)  also  effectively  hyperpolarize  the  membrane  of 
the  initial  segment.  However  the  currents  generated 
by  this  ionic  mechanism  are  even  more  effective  in 
checking  depolarization  (18).  On  this  account,  with 
any  of  the  three  methods  of  stimulation,  synaptic, 
direct  or  antidromic,  there  is  an  increased  difficulty  in 
generating  an  impulse  in  the  motoneuron.  All  the 
various  types  of  inhibitory  action  can  be  sufficiently 
explained  by  the  increased  ionic  conductance  pro- 
duced by  the  inhibitory  transmitter  substance  and  the 
consequent  flow  of  postsynaptic  currents  that  oppo.se 
the  excitatory  currents  [fig.  8;  cf  Coombs  et  al.  (18); 
Eccles  (29)]. 

The  low  threshold  of  the  initial  segment  relative  to 


the  soma-dendritic  membrane  accounts  for  the  ob- 
servation that  with  normal  motoneurons  impulses  are 
always  generated  in  the  initial  segment.  As  a  conse- 
quence the  motoneuron  acts  as  a  far  better  integrator 
of  the  whole  synaptic  e.xcitatory  and  inhibitory  bom- 
bardment than  would  be  the  case  if  impulses  were 
generated  anywhere  over  the  whole  soma-dendritic 
membrane.  If  these  latter  conditions  obtained,  a 
special  strategic  grouping  of  excitatory  synapses  [cf 
Lorente  de  No  (65)]  could  initiate  an  impulse  despite 
a  relative  paucity  of  the  total  excitatory  synaptic 
bombardment  and  a  considerable  inhibitory  bom- 
bardment of  areas  remote  from  this  focus.  As  it  is, 
both  e.xcitatory  and  inhibitory  synaptic  action  are 
effective  onlv  in  so  far  as  they  affect  the  membrane 
potential  of  the  initial  segment.  It  is  here  that  the 
conflict  between  excitation  and  inhibition  is  joined, 
not  generally  over  the  motoneuronal  surface,  as  was 
envisaged  by  Sherrington  in  his  concept  of  algebraic 
summation. 

In  the  account  so  far  given  the  soma-dendritic 
surface  functions  merely  as  a  generating  area  for  the 
postsynaptic  currents  that  are  eff"ective  only  in  so  far 
as  they  act  on  the  initial  segment  either  in  generating 
an  impulse  or  in  preventing  it.  If  an  impulse  so 
generated  invades  the  soma-dendritic  membrane,  it 
does  so  after  the  discharge  has  occurred  along  the 
axon  (14).  It  might  thus  appear  that  the  invasion  of 
the  soma-dendritic  membrane  is  of  no  consequence  in 
the  essential  function  of  the  neuron  in  discharging 
impulses  down  its  axon.  However,  in  contrast  to  the 
initial  segment  and  the  medullated  axon  of  neurons, 
the  soma-dendritic  membrane  of  many  species  of 
neurons  develops  after  an  impulse  a  large  and  pro- 
longed after-hyperpolarization  (15,  68).  This  after- 
hyperpolarization  delays  the  generation  of  the  next 
impulse  by  the  neuron  and  thus  very  eflTectively  slows 
the  frequency  of  the  rhythmic  discharges  of  neurons 
[cf  Eccles  (28),  pp.  174-8].  This  frequency  control 
by  the  soma-dendritic  membrane  is  \ery  important  in 
limiting  the  frequency  with  which  motoneurons 
activate  muscles.  Recently  it  has  jjcen  shown  that  the 
motoneurons  supplying  the  slow  postural  muscles  ha\e 
much  more  prolonged  after-hyperpolarizations  than 
those  supplying  the  fast  phasic  mu.scles  (30). 

Central  Inhibitory  Patliivays 

It  may  be  taken  as  established  that  at  least  some 
afferent  fibers,  e.g.  those  from  annulospiral  endings 
and  tendon  organs,  act  as  pathways  both  for  excita- 
torv  and  inhibitory  actions  on  motoneurons,  and  in 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION 


addition  exert  excitatory  actions  directly  on  other 
neurons  in  the  spinal  cord  (31,  32,  33,  35,  61,  62). 
Until  recently  values  for  the  central  conduction  time 
of  the  so-called  direct  inhibitory  pathway  (annulo- 
spiral  afferent  fibers  to  motoneurons  of  antagonist 
action)  were  derived  by  measurements  of  the  shortest 
interval  at  which  an  inhibitory  volley  can  precede  a 
monosynaptic  excitatory  volley  and  yet  be  eflfective  in 
inhibiting  the  reflex  discharge.  Since  such  intervals 
approximated  to  zero,  it  was  erroneously  concluded 
that  the  latency  of  direct  inhibitory  action  approxi- 
mated to  that  of  monosynaptic  excitatory  action,  and 
hence  that  the  inhibitory  pathway  was  also  monosyn- 
aptic, i.e.  that  the  annulospiral  afTerents  of  muscle 
had  inhibitory  synaptic  endings  on  motoneurons  (5, 
28,  61,  64).  However  the  IPSP  generated  under  such 
conditions  has  a  latent  period  at  least  0.8  msec. 
longer  than  the  monosynaptic  excitatory  action  of  a 
comparable  pathway  (35),  which  is  just  the  interval 
that  would  be  expected  if  there  were  a  synaptic  relay 
on  the  inhibitory  pathway.  It  has  further  been  found 
that  the  annulospiral  afTerents  establish  a  synaptic 
relay  in  the  interinediate  nucleus  which  conforms  in 
every  respect  with  the  properties  of  the  direct  inhibi- 
tory pathway  (35)-  Of  particular  significance  is  the 
recent  observation  that  the  summed  action  of  im- 
pulses in  several  annulospiral  fibers  is  required  before 
any  IPSP  is  produced  by  them,  which  contrasts  with 
their  monosynaptic  excitatory  pathway,  where  the 
individual  impulses  are  independently  effective  in 
generating  EPSP  (36).  Evidently  the  spatial  summa- 
tion of  the  inhibitory  impulses  also  requires  the 
synaptic  relay  station  that  has  been  found  in  the 
intermediate  nucleus  and  that  is  required  in  explain- 
ing the  long  central  latency  of  the  'direct'  inhibitory 
pathway.  The  same  additional  latency  and  inter- 
neuronal  relay  are  observed  for  the  IPSP  generated 
through  the  contralateral  inhibitory  pathway  which 
Wilson  &  Lloyd  (78)  have  discovered  in  the  Sj  and 
S3  segmental  levels  (20).  Finally,  the  monosynaptic 
excitatory  action  of  afferent  impulses  from  the  quadri- 
ceps and  gracilis  muscles  on  soleus  and  biceps-semi- 
tendinosus  motoneurons,  respectively,  (32)  provides  a 
sufficient  explanation  of  Sprague's  observation  (74) 
that  some  afferent  fibers  entering  by  the  L5  dorsal 
root  establish  synaptic  connections  directly  with 
motoneurons  of  the  L7  and  Si  segments  [cf.  Eccles 
(29),  p.  156].  It  may  therefore  be  taken  as  established 
that  a  single  interneuron  is  interpolated  on  the  direct 
inhibitory  pathway,  as  shown  diagrammatically  in 
figure  g.-l.  Similarly  there  is  a  single  interneuron  on 
the  inhibitory  pathway  from  motor  axon  collaterals 


to  motoneurons  (34),  as  is  shown  diagrammatically 
in  figure  gfi.  By  a  systematic  study  of  the  IPSP's  pro- 
duced by  afferent  impulses  in  the  fibers  of  Golgi 
tendon  organs,  it  has  recently  been  found  that  there  is 
always  at  least  one  interneuron  on  the  inhibitory 
pathway,  though  sometimes  two  are  interpolated  (33)- 

Inhibitory  and  Excitatory  Transmitter  Substances 

Strychnine  has  been  found  to  have  a  highly  specific 
and  rapid  action  in  depressing  inhibitory  synaptic 
action  (cf.  Grundfest,  Chapter  V,  fig.  12),  at  least 
with  the  five  types  of  inhibitory  action  that  have  so 
far  been  investigated  in  the  spinal  cord  (5,  18,  29). 
Similarly,  tetanus  toxin  very  effectively  depresses  all 
these  inhibitory  synaptic  actions  (9).  In  fact  the 
clinical  effects  of  both  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin 
can  be  sufficiently  explained  by  these  actions.  Since 
the  activation  of  the  inhibitory  interneurons  is  not 
affected  when  synaptic  inhibitory  action  has  been 
virtually  abolished  by  strychnine  or  tetanus  toxin,  it 
may  be  concluded  that  these  agents  exert  their  de- 
pressant action  on  the  inhibitory  synapses,  as  indi- 
cated in  figure  9.4  and  B.  On  account  of  the  rapidity 
and  effectiveness  of  its  action  it  seems  likely  that 
strychnine  acts  competitively  with  the  inhibitory 
transmitter  for  the  receptor  patches  of  the  inhibitory 
subsynaptic  membrane.  Certainly  the  highly  specific 
actions  of  tetanus  toxin  and  strychnine  indicate  that 
inhibitory  synaptic  action  is  mediated  by  a  specific 
inhibitory  transmitter  substance. 

The  interneuron  on  the  inhibitory  pathways  (cf. 
fig.  9.4  and  E)  can  be  regarded  as  being  introduced  in 
order  to  change  over  from  a  neuron  that  manufactures 
and  liberates  an  excitatory  transmitter  substance  to 
one  that  operates  through  the  inhibitory  transmitter 
substance.  It  is,  therefore,  postulated  that  any  one 
transmitter  substance  always  has  the  same  synaptic 
action,  i.e.  excitatory  or  inhibitory,  at  all  synapses  on 
nerve  cells  in  the  mammalian  central  nervous  system. 
According  to  this  principle,  any  one  class  of  nerve 
cells  in  the  mammalian  central  nervous  system  will 
function  exclusively  either  in  an  excitatory  or  in  an 
inhibitory  capacity  at  all  of  its  synaptic  endings,  i.e. 
it  is  postulated  that  there  are  functionally  just  two 
types  of  nerve  cells,  excitatory  and  inhibitory.  The 
interneurons  illustrated  in  figure  9.4  and  B  are  ex- 
amples of 'inhibitory  neurons'.  On  the  other  hand, 
the  dorsal  root  ganglion  cells  with  their  primary 
afferent  fibers,  proi^ably  the  neurons  of  all  the  long 
tracts  both  ascending  and  descending,  the  moto- 
neurons, and  many  interneurons  belong  to  the  class 


72 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


/f 


■  Blocked  b/  dihydfo-^-erythroi'j  rt 


ToT     "^V 


BLOCKED     BY     STRYCHNINE,     TETANUS      TOXIN 


MOTO 
NEURONE 


FIG.  g.  A.  Schematic  drawing  of  tiie  anatomical  and  physiological  features  of  the  direct  inhibitory 
pathway.  It  shows  the  events  in  the  primary  afferent  hber,  in  its  excitatory  synaptic  connections  with 
an  intermediate  neuron  (I  cell)  and  finally  in  the  inhibitory  synaptic  connection  of  this  neuron  with  a 
motoneuron,  where  the  inhibitory  subsynaptic  current  is  shown  by  a  broken  line  and  the  IPSP  by 
a  continuous  line  (cf.  fig.  6A).  B.  Diagram  summarizing  the  postulated  sequence  of  events  from  an 
impulse  in  a  motor  axon  to  the  inhibition  of  a  motoneuron.  All  events  are  plotted  on  the  time  scale 
shown  below  and  the  corresponding  histological  structures  are  shown  diagrammatically  to  the  left 
(note  indicator  arrows).  The  four  plotted  time  courses  are  from  above  downwards  for  the  following 
events:  the  electrical  response  of  impulse  in  motor-axon  collateral;  the  electrical  response  evoked  in  a 
Renshaw  cell  by  the  cumulatixe  effect  of  acetylcholine  at  many  synapses,  showing  impulses  super- 
imposed on  a  background  depolarization;  the  IPSP  generated  in  the  motoneuron  by  the  Renshaw 
cell  discharge;  and  the  aggregate  IPSP  evoked  in  a  motoneuron  that  is  bombarded  repetitively  by 
many  Renshaw  cells,  which  become  progressively  more  asynchronous,  so  smoothing  the  latter  part 
of  the  ripple.  The  structural  diagram  to  the  left  shows  converging  synapses  on  the  Renshaw  cell 
and  on  the  motoneuron.  [From  Eccles  c/  at.  C34)-j 


'excitatory  neurons'.  Conceptually,  by  this  subdivision 
of  nerve  cells  into  excitatory  and  inhibitory  types,  a 
great  simplification  is  produced  in  the  physiology  of 
central  synaptic  mechanisms,  for  all  branches  of  any 
one  neuron  can  be  regarded  as  having  the  same 
synaptic  function,  i.e.  as  being  uniformly  excitatory 
or  uniformly  inhibitory.  Terzuolo  &  Bullock  (75) 
give  experimental  evidence  that  this  principle  of 
neuronal  specificity  does  not  hold  for  the  cardiac 
ganglion  of  Limulus. 

In  attempting  to  understand  the  operation  of  any 
neuronal  system  in  the  mainmalian  central  nervous 
system,  a  useful  provisional  postulate  would  be  that 
all  inhibitory  cells  are  short-axon  neurons  lying  in  the 
grey  matter,  while  all  transmission  pathways  including 
the  peripheral  afferent  and  efferent  pathways  are 
formed  by  the  axons  of  excitatory  cells.  Such  a  postu- 
late would  be  of  most  direct  application  in  relation  to 
such  simple  problems  as  the  modes  of  termination  of 
the  descending  tracts,  but  eventually  it  may  be 
applicable  also  to  more  complex  situations  in  the 
brainstem  and  even  in  the  cerebellar  and  cerebral 
cortices.  In  all  these  situations  there  is  as  yet  no  infor- 


mation on   the  structural  features  of  the  inhibitory 
mechanisms. 

It  will  be  sufficiently  evident  from  the  above 
account  of  nerve  cells  that  interactions  between  nerve 
cells  are  attributed  to  synaptic  contacts  which  operate 
by  a  specific  chemical  transmitter  mechanism.  The 
alternate  postulate  is  that,  at  least  in  part,  interaction 
between  neurons  is  attributable  to  the  flow  of  electric 
currents  generated  by  active  neurons.  There  is  at 
present  no  experimental  evidence  that  the  nervous 
system  ot  \ertebrates  operates  in  this  way.  The  flow 
of  electric  currents  between  neurons  is  far  too  small 
to  have  any  significant  effect,  even  in  experiments 
using  the  unphysiologicai  procedure  of  large  syn- 
chronous volleys.  In  contrast  it  should  be  mentioned 
that  some  synapses  in  Crustacea  do  operate  by  elec- 
trical transmission,  there  being  special  permeai:)ility 
and  rectification  properties  of  the  apposed  synaptic 
memijranes  (47).  Such  a  mechanism  would  have  been 
detected  if  it  were  operative  at  any  of  the  central 
synapses  of  vertebrates  that  have  been  systematically 
investigated. 


NEURON    PHYSIOLOGY INTRODUCTION 


73 


REFERENCES 


1.  Araki,  T.  and  T.  Otani.  J.  Neurophysiol.  i8:  472,  1955. 

2.  Bernstein,  J.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  92:  521,  1902. 

3.  BoDiAN,  D.  Cold  Spiitiii  Harbor  Symp.  Qjianl.  Biol.  17:  I,  1952. 

4.  BoDiAN,  D.  AND  R.  C.  Mellors.  J.  Exper.  Med.  81 :  469, 

1945- 

5.  Bradley,  K.,  D.  M.  Easton  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 
122:474,  1953. 

6.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 

i'T-  43'.  '952- 

7.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 
122:429,  1953. 

8.  Brooks,  C.  McC,  J.  C.  Eccies  and  J.  L.  Malcolm.  J. 
.Meurophysiol.   11:417,   1948. 

9.  Brooks,  V.  B.,  D.  R.  Curtis  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 
>35-  655,  1957. 

I  o.  Burns,  B.  D.  J.  Physiol.  1 1 1  :  50,  1 950. 

I  1 .   Cajal,  S.   R.    Trab.  Lab.  Invest.  Biol.   Univ.  Madrid  29 :   i . 

'934- 

12.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  D.  R.  Curtis  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  Nature, 
London  178:  1049,  1956. 

13.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  D.  R.  Curtis  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 

■  39:  igfi.  1957- 

14.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  D.  R.  Curtis  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 
139:232,  1957. 

15.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  .and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.  130: 

29  >>  1955- 

16.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.  130: 

326,  195.5- 

17.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.   130: 

374.  1955- 

18.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C,   Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.   130: 

396,  1955- 

19.  CouTEAU.x,   R.   AND  J.   Taxi.   Anh.   Anat.   Microsc.   Morph. 
Exper.  41:  352,   1952. 

19a.  Curtis,   D.   R.,  J.   C.   Eccles  and   .\.   Lundberg.   Acta 
physiol.  scandinav.  In  press. 

20.  Curtis,   D.   R.,   K.   Krnjevic   and   R.   Miledi.  J.  Neuro- 
physiol.  21:  319,  1958. 

21.  DE  Robertis,  E.  D.  P.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem.  Cytol.  2:  503, 

■956. 

22.  DE  Robertis,  IC.  D.  P.  and  H.  S.  Bennett.  J.  Bwphrs.  & 
Biochem.  Cytol.   1:  47,   1955. 

23.  DE  Robertis,  E.  D.  P.  and  C.  M.  Franchi.  J.  Biophts.  & 
Biochem.  Cytol.  2:  307,  1956. 

24.  DEL  Castillo,  ].  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  125:  546,  1954. 

25.  DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  129:  48P,  1955. 

26.  DEL  Castillo,  J.   and  B.  Katz.   Prog.  Biophys.  &  Biophys. 
Chem.  6:  121,  1956. 

27.  Diamond,  J.,  J.  A.  B.  Gray  and  M.  Sato.  J.  Physiol.  133: 

54.  >956. 

28.  Eccles,  J.    C.    The  Neurophysiological   Basis  0/  Mmd:    The 
Principles  of  .Neurophysiology.  Oxford;  Clarendon  Press,  1953. 

29.  Eccles,   J.    C.    The    Physiology   oj   Nerve    Cells.    Baltimore: 
Johns  Hopkins  Press,  1957,  270  pp. 

30.  Eccles,  J.   C,   R.  M.   Eccles  and  .\.   Lundberg.  .Nature, 
London  179:  866,  1957. 

31.  Eccles,  J.  C,  R.  M.  Eccles  and  A.  Lundberg.  J.  Physiol. 

■36:527.  '957- 

32.  Eccles,  J.  C,  R.  M.  Eccles  and  A.  Lundberg.  J.  Physiol. 
137:  22,  1957. 


33.  Eccles,  J.  C,  R.  M.  Eccles  and  A.  Lundberg.  J.  Physiol. 
138:  227,  1957. 

34.  Ecci-ES,  J.  C,  P.  Fatt  and  K.  Koketsu.  J.  Physiol.   126: 
524.  1954- 

35.  Eccles,  J.  C,  P.  Fatt  and  S.  Landgren.  J.  Neurophysiol. 
19:  75.  1956- 

36.  Eccles,  R.  M.  and  A.  Lundberg.  Nature,  London  179:  1305, 

'957- 

37.  Edwards,  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  Fed.  Proc.  16:  34,  1957. 

38.  Fatt,  P.  Physiol.  Rev.  34-  674,  1954. 

39.  Fatt,  P.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  27,  1957. 

40.  Fatt,  P.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  61,  1957. 

41.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  115:  320,  1951. 

42.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  121 :  374,  1953. 

43.  Feldberg,  W.  Physiol.  Rev.  25:  596,   1945. 

44.  Frank,  K.   and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes.  J.  Physiol.    134:  451, 
■956. 

45.  Frank,  K.  and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes.  Fed.  Proc.  16:  39,  1957. 

46.  Fuortes,   M.   G.   F.,  K.   Frank  and  M.   C.   Becker.  J. 
Gen.  Physiol.  40.  735,   1957. 

47.  Furshpan,   E.  J.   AND  D.   D.   Potter.  Nature,  London   180: 
342,  1957- 

48.  Geren,   B.   B.    AND   F.   A.   Schmitt.   Proc.  .Nat.   Acad.   Sc, 
Wash.  40:  863,  1954. 

49.  Hodgkin,  .\.  L.  Biol.  Rev.  26,  339,   1951. 

50.  Hodgkin,  A.  L.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  148;  i,  1958. 

51.  Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  P.   Horowicz.  J.   Physiol.    137:   30P, 

'957- 

52.  Hodgkin,  .\.  L.  and  .\.  F.  Huxley.  J.  Physiol.  117:  500, 

'952- 

53.  Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  R.  D.  Keynes.  J.  Physiol.   128:  28, 

'955- 

54.  Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  R.  D.  Keynes.  J.  Physiol.   131:  592, 

■9.56. 

55.  Howland,   B,    J.   V.   Lettvin,   VV.   S.   McCulloch,   VV. 
Pitts  and  P.  D.  Wall.  J.  Neurophysiol.   18:  i,  1955. 

56.  Hutter,  O.  F.  and  VV.  Trautwein.  J.  Physiol.   129:  48P, 

'955- 

57.  HuXLE-i',  A.  F.  In  :  Ion  Transport  across  Memhianes.  New  York  : 
Acad.  Press,  1954. 

58.  H^'DEN,  H.  Acta  physiol.  scindmav.  6.  suppl.   17,   1943. 

59.  Katz,  B.  Arch.  Sc.  Physiol.  3:  285,  1949. 

60.  Kuffler,  S.  VV.  and  C.  Eyzaguirre.  J.  Gen.  Physiol  39; 

155.  1955- 

61.  Laporte,  Y.  and  D.  p.  C.  Lloyd.  Am.  J.  PhysioL  169:  609, 

'952- 

62.  Laporte,    Y.,    A.    Lundberg    and    O.    Oscarsson.    Acta 
physiol.  scindinav.  36:  188,  1956. 

63.  LiLEY,  A.  VV.  J.  Physiol.  134:  427,  1956. 

64.  Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  9:  421,  1946. 

65.  Lorente  DE  N6,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.   i  :  195,   1938. 

66.  Palade,  G.  E.  and  S.  L.  Palay.  Anat.  Rec.  1 18:  335,  1954. 

67.  Palay,  S.  L.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem.  Cytol.  Suppl.,  2:  193, 
■956. 

68.  Phillips,  C.  G.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  41  :  58,  1956. 

69.  Reger,  J.  F.  Anat.  Rec.  122:  i,  1955. 

70.  Robertson,  J.  D.  j'.  Bio/iAj'i.  G?  BiocAcm.  C>'(o/.  2:  381,  1956. 

71.  Sherrington,    C.    S.    In  A    Text   Book  oj  Physiology  (7th 
ed.)    edited  by  M.  Foster.  London:  Macmillan,   1897. 

72.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  97:  519, 
1925- 


74  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -"  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 

73.  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  105:  332,  76.  Trautwein,  W.,  S.  W.   Kufflf.r  and  C.   Edwards.  J. 
1929.  Gen.  Physiol.  40:  135,  1956. 

74.  Sprague,  J.  M.  XX  Inlernat.  Physio'..  Congr.,  Abstr.  of  Com-  77.  Weidmann,  S.  J.  Physiol.  1 14:  372,  1951. 

munic. :  849,  1 956.  78.  Wilson,  V.  J.  and  D.  P.  C.  Lloyd.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  1 87 :  641 , 

75.  Terzuolo,  C.  a.  and  T.  H.  Bullock.  Arch.  ital.  biol.  96;  1956- 

117,  1958.  79-  Young,  J.  Z.  Endeavour  15:  5,  1956. 


CHAPTER    III 


Conduction  of  the  nerve  impulse 


ICHIJ  I   TASAKI 


Laboratory  of  Neurophysiology,  National  Institute  oj  Neurological  Diseases  and  Blindness, 
National  Institutes  of  Health,  Bethesda,  Maryland 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Introduction 

Compound  Character  of  Peripheral  Nerve 
General  Character  of  the  Nerve  Impulse 
Cable  Properties  of  the  Invertebrate  Axon 
Cable  Properties  of  the  Myelinated  Nerve  Fiber 
Conductance  of  the  Membrane  During  Activity 
Threshold  and  Subthreshold  Phenomena 

Threshold  Membrane  Potential 

Strength-Duration  Relation 

Subthreshold  Response 

Measurement    of    Excitability     by    Using     Test    Shocks 
Abolition  of  the  Action  Potential 
Nervous  Conduction  Along  Uniform  Axon 

Nervous   Conduction   in   Myelinated    Nerve   Fiber   (Saltatory 
Conduction) 

Effect  of  Increase  of  External  Resistance 

Safety  Factor 

Does  the  Nerve  Impulse  Jump  from  Node  to  Node? 

Field  of  Potential  Produced  by  a  Nerve  Impulse 

Conduction  in  a  Polarized  Nerve  Fiber 

Pfliiger's  Law  of  Contraction 

Effect  of  Narcosis  upon  Nervous  Conduction 
After-Potentials  and  Rhythmical  Activity 

After-Potentials 

Rhythmical  Activity 
Current  Theories  of  the  Resting  and  Action   Potentials 

Resting  Potential 

Action  Potential 


THE  MODERN  DEVELOPMENT  of  the  coiicept  of  thc  neivc 
impulse  may  be  said  to  have  started  with  the  measure- 
ment of  the  velocity  of  the  nerve  impulse  by  von 
Helmholtz  (141)  in  1850.  He  measured  the  time 
interval  between  delivery  of  an  electric  shock  to  the 
nerve  of  a  nerve-muscle  preparation  and  the  start 
of  contraction  of  the  muscle  by  two  different  methods. 
The  first  method  used  was  to  start  a  constant  current 
throuE;h    a    ballistic    galvanometer    at    the    lime    of 


delivery  of  the  shock  and  to  interrupt  the  current 
automatically  by  a  switch  opened  by  the  twitch  of 
the  muscle.  The  second  method  he  used  was  based 
on  graphical  registration  of  the  muscular  contraction 
on  a  moving  surface.  He  compared  the  time  intervals 
measured  by  stimulating  the  nerve  near  its  two  ex- 
treme ends. 

Helmholtz's  finding  and  the  subsequent  con- 
firmation and  expansion  of  his  observation  by  a 
number  of  investigators  established  the  fact  that  a 
nerve  impulse  travels  along  the  nerve  at  a  rate  far 
slower  than  that  of  light  or  sound  in  a  similar  medium 
but  substantially  faster  than  the  process  of  transporta- 
tion of  substances  by  streaming  or  diffusion  in  a  slender 
tube  like  a  nerve  fiber.  Later,  in  1908,  Lucas  (80) 
found  that  the  velocity  of  the  nerve  impulse  doubles 
with  a  rise  in  temperature  of  about  10  degrees.  The 
question  of  whether  or  not  the  nerve  impulse  is 
associated  with  any  chemical  reacdons,  however, 
was  not  solved  until  Tashiro  (138),  Parker  (98), 
Fenn  (32)  and  Gerard  (40)  established  the  increase 
in  production  of  carbon  dioxide  and  consumption  of 
oxygen  related  to  nervous  activity.  The  demonstra- 
tion of  heat  production  associated  with  propagation 
of  nerve  impulses  by  Downing  et  al.  (24)  gave  a 
further  strong  support  to  the  view  that  chemical 
reactions  underlie  the  process  of  nervous  conduction.' 

An  entirely  different  line  of  approach  to  the  study 
of  the  processes  underlying  nervous  conduction 
originates  with  Hermann  (47).  He  worked  on  a 
'core-conductor  model'  of  nerve  which  is  the  prede- 
cessor of  the  passive  iron  model  (75).  The  basic  idea 

'  Some  investigators  are  of  the  opinion  that  all  the  chemical 
reactions  take  place  late  in  the  recovery  phase  and  not  during 
the  period  in  which  electrical  signs  of  activity  can  be  observed 
[e.g.  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (59)]. 


75 


76 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


developed  from  the  observations  on  the  model  is  that 
nervous  conduction  may  l^e  mediated  by  a  flow  of 
electric  current  between  successive  portions  of  the 
nerve,  i.e.  by  local  circuits.  Through  very  extensixe 
investigations  of  bioelectricity  by  Matteucci  (86), 
Du  Bois-Reymond  (25),  Biedermann  (12)  and  others, 
it  became  known  that  a  transient  potential  variation 
is  generated  by  a  stimulation  of  a  nerve  between  the 
portion  of  the  nerve  or  the  muscle  carrying  an  impulse 
and  the  killed  or  resting  portion.  The  existence  of  a 
local  circuit  is  therefore  a  logical  consequence  of  the 
direct  observations  on  the  bioelectricity  of  the  ner\e. 

A  direct  demonstration  of  the  decisive  role  played 
bv  a  local  circuit  in  the  propaeation  of  an  impulse 
was  brought  forward,  a  long  time  after  Hermann's 
prediction,  first  by  Osterhout  &  Hill  (95)  who  worked 
not  on  the  nerve  but  on  a  large  plant  cell,  Xitella. 
They  found  that  propagation  of  an  impulse  along  this 
elongated  cell  can  be  reversibly  blocked  under  certain 
experimental  conditions  by  removing  or  reconnecting 
a  salt  bridge  which  constituted  a  part  of  the  local 
circuit.  Later,  similar  obsersations  were  made  both 
on  isolated  invertebrate  nerve  fibers  (52)  [cf.  also  (50)] 
and  on  single  nerve  fibers  of  the  toad  (i  17). 

The  development  of  the  concept  of  the  all-or-none 
relationship  between  the  intensity  of  stimulus  and 
the  'size  of  the  response'  followed  a  long,  confusing 
course.  In  1871,  Bowditch  (16)  found  that  the 
magnitude  of  contraction  in  an  excised  heart  muscle 
of  the  frog  is  independent  of  the  intensity  of  the  shock 
used;  a  weak  shock,  if  effective  at  all,  causes  a  con- 
traction which  is  as  large  as  that  caused  by  a  strong 
shock.  A  similar  quantal  relationship  between  the 
twitch  and  stimulus  intensity  was  demonstrated  in 
individual  muscle  fibers  of  the  frog  sartorius  muscle 
(loi)  and  also  in  a  ner\-e-mu.scle  preparation  of  the 
frog  containing  a  small  number  of  nerve  fibers  (81). 
In  these  cases  the  'size  of  the  response'  represents 
the  magnitude  of  muscular  contraction  observed  at 
some   distance    away   from    the   site   of  stimulation. 

Attempting  to  expand  the  concept  of  'size  of 
response'  to  include  the  response  in  the  nerve  itself, 
Lucas  (82)  and  Adrian  (i)  introduced  the  idea  of 
measuring  the  nerve  impulse  by  its  ability  to  stimu- 
late the  adjacent  portion  of  nerve,  or  by  its  capability- 
to  travel  across  a  narcotized  region  of  nerve — the 
logic  being  analogous  to  measuring  the  power  of  a 
man  by  his  ability  to  cross  a  desert.  Through  a  num- 
ber of  ingenious  experiments,  Lucas  and  Adrian 
concluded  that  the  size  of  the  nerve  impulse  in  in- 
dividual nerve  fibers  was  independent  of  the  way 
it  was  elicited.  Kato  (69)  and  his  associates  and  also 


Da\is  ('/  al.  (23)  pointed  out  that  there  was  an  er- 
roneous assumption  in  this  argument  as  to  the 
mechanism  of  narcotic  action.  However,  the  con- 
clusion that  a  propagated  ner\'c  impulse  obeyed  the 
all-or-none   law   turned  out   to   be  perfectly  correct. 

Another  .series  of  somewhat  controversial  argu- 
ments was  evoked  among  investigators  when  the 
concept  of  'local'  or  'subthreshold'  response  was 
introduced  in  physiology.  In  1937  Rushton  (105) 
predicted  the  existence  of  a  local  response  in  nerve 
by  the  following  argument.  If  propagation  of  a  nerve 
impulse  is  due  to  successive  stimulation  of  a  resting 
portion  of  ner\-e  by  the  neighboring  active  (respond- 
ing) area,  a  definite  minimum  area  of  a  nerve  has 
to  be  excited  by  the  stimulating  current  in  order  that 
the  response  at  the  site  of  stimulation  can  generate  a 
propagated  all-or-none  response.  In  other  words, 
he  stipulates  that  there  should  be  a  'response'  at 
the  site  of  stimulation  that  is  too  small  to  initiate  a 
full  sized  propagating  response. 

.Soon  after  Rushton's  prediction,  Hodgkin  (51) 
obtained  clear-cut  records  indicating  the  existence 
of  'subthreshold  responses'  in  the  invertebrate  nerve 
fiber.  However,  it  was  found  later  that  Hodgkin's 
demonstration  did  not  prove  the  legitimac\  of 
Rushton's  argument.  Cole  &  Curtis  (19)  proved  that 
the  resistance  of  the  surface  membrane  of  the  squid 
nerve  fiber  decreases  at  the  peak  of  its  response 
to  about  '200  "'  the  resistance  at  rest;  a  responding 
area  of  the  squid  axon  behaves  like  a  battery  with  no 
appreciable  internal  resistance.  Lender  ordinary 
experimental  conditions,  it  is  practically  impossible 
to  elicit  a  full-sized  response  in  an  area  too  small  to 
initiate  a  propagated  impulse.  Furthermore,  these 
subthreshold  respon.ses  were  demonstrated  in  sc)uid 
axons  of  which  a  large  area  was  subjected  uniformly 
to  a  stimulating  current.  Later  we  shall  discuss 
similar  phenomena  obser\-ed  in  the  nixclinaied  nerve 
fiber  (p.  98! 

We  have  discussed  up  to  this  poiiu  the  coin-se  of 
development  of  some  of  the  basic  concepts  concerning 
the  nature  of  the  nerve  impulse.  We  shall  describe 
on  the  following  pages  the  main  experimental  facts 
known  ai)Oul  the  nerve  filler  and  its  ai)ilit\'  to  carry 
impulses.  Emphasis  will  be  placed  on  the  data  ob- 
tained from  in\ertebrate  and  vertebrate  single  nerve 
fibers.  There  is  good  reason  to  belie\e  that,  at  least 
in  this  field  of  physiology,  the  iiehavior  of  an  as- 
sembh'  of  many  nervous  elements  can  be  understood 
if  the  beha\ior  of  indi\idual  fibers  under  simple, 
well-defined,  experimental  conditions  is  known. 
It  is  generally  extremely  difiicult  to  infer  the  behavior 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


77 


of  individual  fibers  from  observations  on  the  nerve 
trunk. 


COMPOUND   CHARACTER  OF  PERIPHERAL  NERVE 

Soon  after  the  first  World  War,  Forbes  &  Thacher 
(34)  introduced  a  condenser-coupled  vacuum  tube 
amplifier  into  the  field  of  electrophysiology.  Aided 
by  the  continued  development  of  electronic  engi- 
neering, Gasser  &  Erlanger  (38)  in  1922  took  the 
first  photograph  of  a  ner\e  response  recorded  with 
an  instrument  ideal  in  being  inertialess.  They 
started  using  a  cathode  ray  oscillograph  to  register 
the  time  course  of  responses  of  the  nerve. 

The  standard  technique  of  recording  electric  signs 
of  activity  of  a  whole  nerve  trunk  is  to  kill  (ordinarily 
by  crushing)  one  end  of  a  nerve  and  to  place  one 
of  the  recording  electrodes  on  this  killed  end  (see 
fig.  i^).  The  other  electrode  needed  to  measure  the 
potential  difference  is  placed  on  the  intact  part  of 
the  nerve  near  the  killed  end.  Ordinarily,  either  lightly 
chlorided  siKcr  wire  (abbreviated  as  Ag-AgCl) 
or  calomel  half  cells  (Hg-HgCl)  are  used  for  recording 
for  they  are  nonpolarizable.  Stimulating  electrodes 
(S  in  fig.  i)  can  be  either  the  Ag-AgCl  Ringer  type 
or  a  pair  of  plain  platinum  wires.  A  precaution  has 
to  be  taken  to  '  isolate'  the  stimulus  from  ground, 
namely,  to  eliminate  metallic  connection  of  the 
stimulating  electrodes  with  ground.  The  main  reason 
for  the  necessity  of  stimulus  isolation  is  to  prevent 
flow  of  stimulating  (and  other)  currents  between  the 
stimulating  and  ground  electrodes.  The  electrodes 
and  the  nerve  are  generally  mounted  in  a  moist 
chamber  to  prevent  evaporation  of  water  from  the 
surface  of  the  nerve. 

The  arrangement  of  the  recording  electrodes  just 
described  is  called  a  'monophasic  lead'  and  a  re- 
sponse of  the  nerve  recorded  with  this  arrangement  is 
referred  to  as  a  'monophasic  action  potential'.  The 
traditional  picture  illustrating  the  principle  of  this 
method  of  recording  action  potentials  is  as  follows. 
The  portion  of  nerve  carrying  an  impulse  is  'elec- 
trically negative'  to  the  portion  at  rest.  When  an 
impulse  started  by  a  stimulus  emerges  in  the  region 
of  the  recording  electrode  Ei,  the  potential  differ- 
ence between  Ei  and  E2  undergoes  a  transient  vari- 
ation which  makes  the  potential  at  E2  more  positive 
(or  less  negative)  to  that  at  Ei.  Since  the  impulse  does 
not  reach  the  region  of  E2,  a  potential  variation 
representing  the  ner\ous  activity  at  Ei  is  recorded 
monophasically. 


The  modern  picture  illustrating  the  principle  of 
monophasic  recording  (83,  1 24)  is  slightly  different 
from  the  classical  one.  Attention  is  now  focused 
upon  the  nerve  fibers  and  the  intercellular  space  in 
the  nerve  trunk.  When  a  nerve  fiber  carries  an  im- 
pulse, it  generates  a  transient  flow  of  current  in  the 
surrounding  fluid  medium.  In  the  region  of  Ei  and 
E2,  this  transient  current  in  the  intercellular  space  is 
directed  from  E;  to  Ei,  raising  the  potential  at  E2 
relative  to  Ei  for  a  short  period  of  time.  The  currents 
produced  simultaneously  by  many  fibers  in  the 
nerve  are  superposed  in  the  intercellular  space  and 
give  rise  to  a  large  coiTipound  action  potential.  In 
this   modern    picture,    the   'electrical    negati\ity'    in 


40- 


20- 


O.Zmtce 


100 


129 


150-1 


FIG.  I.  A.  Demonstration  of  the  constant  velocity  of 
propagation  of  the  a-  and  /3-waves  in  the  action  potential  of 
the  sciatic  nerve  of  the  bullfrog.  S,  the  stimulating  electrodes; 
El  and  E»,  recording  electrodes,  the  latter  at  the  killed  end 
of  the  nerve.  The  distance  from  the  site  of  stimulation  to  the 
recording  electrode  Ei  is  indicated  on  the  \ertical  line.  The 
starting  points  of  the  oscillograph  trace  show  the  distances  at 
which  the  records  were  taken.  Abscissa,  time.  [From  Gasser  & 
Erlanger  (38).]  B.  A  similar  observation  made  on  a  three- 
fiber  preparation  of  the  toad.  The  diameters  of  the  fibers  were 
13,  9  and  5  II.  The  strength  of  the  stimulating  shocks  employed 
was  twice  the  threshold  for  the  smallest  fiber.  [From  Tasaki 
(124)-] 


78 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  classical  picture-  is  clearly  defined  as  an  IR  drop 
in  the  intercellular  space. 

We  shall  now  proceed  to  discuss  the  time  course  of 
the  action  potential  of  a  buUfroE;  sciatic  nerve  re- 
corded with  this  arrangement.  When  the  distance 
from  the  stimulating;  electrode  (S)  to  the  recording 
electrodes  (Ei)  is  relati\ely  large  and  the  shock  is 
strong  enough  to  stimulate  most  of  the  fibers  in  the 
nerve,  an  action  potential  with  several  separate 
peaks  is  observed  (fig.  i.-!,  bottom).  As  the  distance  is 
altered,  the  time  intervals  between  the  peaks  are 
found  to  alter,  indicating  that  separate  elevations 
in  the  potential  record  represent  processes  travelling 
along  the  nerve  at  different  velocities.  Gasser,  Erlanger, 
Bishop  and  others  (13,  38)  interpreted  these  findings 
as  resulting  from  differences  in  the  conduction  velocity 
of  the  different  fibers  in  the  ner\'c  trunk. 

In  figure  \B,  a  set  of  records  is  presented  showing 
the  validity  of  the  interpretation  just  mentioned. 
Here,  the  connective  tissue  sheath  around  the  sciatic 
nerve  is  removed  near  its  distal  end  and  all  except 
three  nerve  fibers  are  cut  (for  the  detail  of  this  opera- 
tion, cf.  113,  124).  The  two  recording  electrodes 
are  placed  in  two  small  pools  of  Ringer's  solution 
separated  by  a  narrow  air  gap  (o.  i  mm  wide)  across 
which  the  exposed  nerve  fibers  are  mounted.  Under 
these  circumstances,  the  electric  currents  which  the 
nerve  fibers  produce  when  the  impulses  arrive  at  the 
site  of  recording  inevitably  flow  through  the  resistor 
QR  in  the  figure)  connected  between  the  electrodes. 
The  IR  drop  thus  produced  is  amplified  and  recorded 
with  an  oscillograph. 

It  is  seen  in  the  records  that  the  number  of  peaks 
observed  is  equal  to  the  number  of  the  fibers  left 
uncut.  Three  fibers  are  now  generating  three  sepa- 
rate potential  variations.  It  is  also  clear  that  each 
fiber  carries  an  impulse  at  a  rate  which  is  approxi- 
mately constant  for  the  whole  length  of  the  sciatic 
nerve. 

It  is  simple  to  demonstrate  the  statistical  rule 
formulated  by  Erlanger  and  Ga.sser  that  the  con- 
duction velocity  increases  with  increasing  fiber 
diameter.  If  only  one  large  fiber  is  left  uncut,  we 
find  a  high  conduction  velocity;  a  weak  electric 
shock  is  sufficient  to  excite  it.   If  one  small  fiber  is 

-  It  is  important  to  distinguish  a  negative  potential  from  a 
negative  electric  charge.  The  potential  along  a  uniform  electric 
conductor  is  inevitably  related  by  Ohm's  law  to  a  flow  of 
current  in  the  conductor;  it  has  to  be  expressed  as  a  potential 
difference  between  the  two  points  on  the  conductor,  for  in- 
stance, 'the  potential  of  Ei  is  10  mv  below  (or  abose)  that  of 
E;'  but  not   E;  is  negative  and  Ei  is  positive.' 


40  -  m/sec 


30 


20 


10 


o 
o    o 


o  o 


o 

o 


UJ 

> 


z 
o 

I- 
o 
z> 
o 
z 
o 
o 


o 

o 
000 
o 


o 

8    ° 

o 

o   o      o  o 

0 

o 


O    o 


FIBER      DIAMETER 
J I I  I 


8 


10 


12 


14 


16)1 


FIG.  2.  Conduction  velocity  of  individual  nerve  fibers,  V, 
plotted  against  fiber  diameter,  D.  Single  fibers  were  isolated 
from  sciatic-gastrocnemius  preparations  of  the  bullfrog.  The 
outside  diameter  of  the  fiber  was  measured  at  the  operated 
region  near  the  muscle.  Temperature,  24°C.  [From  Tasaki 
et  al.  (l3i)-] 


isolated  in  the  region  of  recording,  we  find  a  small 
response  which  arrives  at  the  site  of  recording  after  a 
long  delay;  a  strong  shock  is  needed  to  stimulate 
such  a  fiber. 

In  figure  2  the  conduction  velocities  of  about  50 
different  fibers  in  the  bullfrog  sciatic  nerve  are  plotted 
against  their  outside  diameter.  There  is  a  rough 
proportionality  between  the  fiber  diameter  and  the 
conduction  velocity,  the  correlation  coefficient  be- 
tween the  two  being  0.92  in  this  measurement.  The 
relation  between  the  minimum  effective  intensity  or 
threshold  of  shock  and  the  fiber  diameter  determined 
by  this  method  can  be  found  elsewhere  (124). 

It  is  well-known  that  the  internodal  length  (the 
distance  between  the  two  neighboring  nodes  of 
Ranvier)  increases  with  the  fiber  diameter.  For  the 
fibers  in  the  bullfrog  sciatic  nerve,  the  relation  be- 
tween the  diameter  D  and  the  internodal  length  L 
was  found  to  be  expressed  by  the  formula 

L  =  0.146  X  io'L», 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


79 


the  correlation  coefficient  between  the  two  being 
0.62.  The  relation  between  the  conduction  velocity 
V  (expressed  in  m  per  sec.)  and  the  diameter  (in  fji) 
presented  in  figure  2  can  be  expressed  by 

V  =  2.50/) 

(at  24°C)-  From  the  two  formulae  abose,  it  follows 
immediately  that 

L 

-   =  0.059  (fnsec). 
I 

The  ratio  L'V  represents  the  average  conduction 
time  for  one  internodal  length.  The  last  expression 
indicates  that,  statistically  speaking,  the  internodal 
conduction  time  is  roughly  independent  of  the  fiber 
diameter. 

In  the  experiments  involving  electric  stimulation  of 
whole  nerve  trunks,  it  is  customary  to  designate 
groups  of  nerve  fibers  of  different  conduction  velocities 
as  a,  /3,  7,  (6),  B  and  C.  Group  a  represents  the 
fastest  myelinated  nerve  fibers  in  the  nerve  with 
velocities  of  20  to  30  m  per  .sec.  in  the  frog,  while  B 
fibers  are  the  slowest  group  (5  m  per  sec.  or  less) 
at  room  temperature.  The  first  three  (or  four)  groups 
are  often  included  in  .1.  Group  C  represents  non- 
myelinated fibers.  This  cla.ssification  is  somewhat 
arbitrary. 

The  distribution  of  the  fiber  sizes  in  a  nerve  trunk 
generally  shows  several  peaks  of  numerical  pre- 
dominance. Reflecting  this  situation,  action  potentials 
recorded  at  some  distance  away  from  the  site  of 
stimulation  develop  sev-eral  peaks.  However,  be- 
cause of  the  difference  in  size  and  duration  of  the 
action  potentials  among  diflferent  fibers,  it  requires  a 
tedious  calculation  to  predict  the  configuration  of 
the  action  potential  of  a  whole  nerve  trunk  on  the 
basis  of  its  fiber  size  distribution.  A  detailed  treat- 
ment of  this  problem  is  found  in  a  monograph  by 
Gasser  &  Erlanger  (38). 


GENERAL  CH.ARACTER  OF  THE  NERVE  IMPULSE 

In  the  preceding  section  we  have  seen  an  example 
of  simplicity  and  clarity  of  the  experiments  done 
with  isolated  single  nerve  fibers.  It  was  Adrian  & 
Bronk  (5)  in  1928  who  made  the  first  successful  at- 
tempt to  reduce  operatively  the  number  of  active 
fibers  in  a  nerve  to  record  single  fiber  responses. 
The  operation  of  isolating  single  nerve  fibers  of  the 
frog  and  the  toad  was  developed  in  Kato's  laboratorv 
(70). 


Another  successful  approach  to  single  fiber  experi- 
ments was  achieved  by  the  use  of  nerve  preparations 
of  invertebrates,  such  as  crabs,  lobsters,  crayfish 
or  squid.  The  operative  procedure  of  obtaining  single 
fibers  in  these  invertebrate  nerves  is  simpler  than  the 
dissection  of  a  single  frog  nerve  fiber,  since  some  of 
the  fibers  in  these  lower  animals  are  larger  than  100 
/i  in  diameter.  So-called  squid  giant  axons,  which 
Young  (146)  has  introduced  to  electrophysiologists, 
are  as  large  as  400  to  900  n  in  diameter  and  are  an 
excellent  material  for  investigating  the  potential 
inside  the  axoplasm. 

Through  the  use  of  single  fiber  preparations,  the 
demonstration  of  some  of  the  basic  properties  of  the 
propagated  nerve  impulse  has  become  extremely 
simple  and  direct.  The  following  properties  are 
common  to  all  the  nerve  fibers  examined,  vertebrate 
and  invertebrate. 

a)  All-or-none  law.  The  historical  aspect  of  the 
development  of  this  law  has  been  mentioned  in  the 
introduction  of  this  chapter.  This  law  may  be  stated 
as  follows:  with  other  factors  constant,  the  size  and 
shape  of  any  electrical  sign  of  a  propagated  nerve 
impulse  is  independent  of  the  intensity  of  stimulus 
employed  to  initiate  the  impulse. 

It  has  been  mentioned  that  a  definite  threshold  in- 
tensity is  needed  to  initiate  an  impulse  in  a  nerve 
fiber.  As  signs  of  an  impulse,  one  may  take  the  current 
de\eloped  by  the  fiber,  the  action  current,  or  the 
potential  changes  inside  the  axoplasm,  or  any  other 
electrical  response  of  the  fiber.  When  the  stimulus 
intensity  is  varied,  these  signs  may  appear  slightly 
earlier  or  later;  but  the  whole  time  course  remains 
uninfluenced  by  how  far  above  threshold  the  stimulus 
intensity  is. 

The  records  presented  in  figure  3  show  the  time 
course  of  the  action  currents  produced  by  a  single 
nerve  fiber  of  a  toad  in  response  to  electric  shocks  of 
varying  intensities.  The  shocks  were  applied  to  the 
sciatic  ner\e  trunk  and  the  current  associated  with 
an  impulse  traveling  along  a  single  nerve  fiber  in  the 
nerve  was  recorded  by  the  technique  described  in  the 
discussion  of  the  experiment  of  figure  iB.  At  threshold 
(the  lowest  trace),  the  action  current  of  the  fiber 
started  after  a  long  and  variable  delay.  The  time 
course  of  this  action  current,  however,  was  identical 
with  that  of  the  other  responses  to  stronger  shocks. 

It  is  possible  to  modify  the  time  course  of  the  electric 
response  of  a  fiber  by  changing  physical  or  chemical 
environmental  conditions,  such  as  temperature  or 
composition  of  the  fluid  around  the  fiber.  This  fact 
should  not  be  regarded  as  a  violation  of  the  all-or- 


8o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


Fio.  3.  Demonstration  of  the  all-or-none  behavior  of  the 
electric  response  (binodal  action  current)  of  a  single  myeli- 
nated fiber.  The  distance  between  the  stimulating  electrodes 
and  the  site  of  recording  was  20  mm.  The  strengths  of  the 
stimulating  shocks  were,  from  the  bottom  upward,  100,  105, 
150,  200,  250,  300  and  350  per  cent  of  threshold,  respectively. 
Time  marker,  5000  cycles  per  sec.  Temperature,  20.5°C. 
[From  Tasaki  (124).] 


none  law.  This  law  refers  to  the  identity  of  the  re- 
sponses obtained  by  changing  only  the  stimulus 
intensity  and  nothing  else. 

The  all-or-none  law  is  not  applicable,  at  least  not 
in  as  strict  a  form  as  described  above,  to  electrical 
responses  recorded  at  the  site  of  stimulation 
(cf.  p.  98). 

/))  The  refractory  period.  The  time  course  of  the 
response  of  a  nerve  fiber  is  not  influenced  bv  the  rate 
at  which  the  stimulating  shocks  arc  repeated  as  long 
as  the  rate  is  less  than  about  10  per  sec.  When,  how- 
ever, the  repetition  rate  is  increased  up  to  about  100 
per  .sec.  at  room  temperature,  it  is  found  that  the 
responses  are  difl'crent  in  their  size  and  shape  from 
the  responses  obtained  at  lower  frequencies.  During 
a  short  period  of  time  after  an  impulse  has  swept  over 
the  fiber,  the  'condition'  of  the  fiber  is  different  from 
that  of  a  normal  resting  filler.  This  period  is  called 
the  refractory  period  of  the  nerve  fiber. 

It  is  customary  to  investigate  the  properties  of  a 
nerve  fiber  in  the  refractory  period  by  using  a  series 
of  paired  stimuli,  a  brief  conditioning  shock  followed 
by  a  brief  test  shock  at  an  adjustable  interval.  The 
response  of  the  fiber  to  the  first  conditioning  shock 
has  the  normal  configuration,  while  the  response  to 
the  second  test  shock  varies  with  the  time  interval 
between  the  paired  shocks.  The  threshold  for  the 
second  shock  is  known  to  undergo  a  pronounced 
change  during  the  early  stage  in  the  refractory  period. 

The    curve    representing    the    time    course    of   the 


gradual  change  in  threshold  with  increasing  shock 
intervals  is  generally  called  a  "recovery  curve'.  In  the 
first  recovery  curve  published  by  Adrian  &  Lucas  (6) 
in  1912,  the  reciprocal  of  threshold,  the 'excitability', 
was  plotted  against  the  interval  between  the  two 
shocks.  The  thick  continuous  line  in  figure  4  shows  a 
recovery  cur\e  determined  by  using  the  propagated 
impulses  of  a  single  nerve  fiber  as  the  index.  The 
threshold  for  the  test  shock  alone  (measured  i  sec.  or 
more  after  the  conditioning  shock)  is  taken  as  unity. 
The  oljserved  data  indicate  that,  as  the  interval  be- 
tween the  conditioning  and  test  shocks  decreases,  the 
threshold  for  the  test  shock  rises  first  gradually  and 
then  more  rapidly.  There  is  a  sharp  break  in  the  curve 
at  the  moment  when  the  threshold  is  about  J. 5  to 
3  times  the  normal  \alue,  namely,  when  the  excita- 
bility is  about  30  to  40  per  cent  of  the  normal  level. 

This  break  in  the  recovery  cur\e  indicates  that,  in 
the  period  following  initiation  of  a  propagated  nerve 
impulse  in  a  nerve  fiber,  there  is  a  definite  period 
during  which  the  fiber  is  incapable  of  carrying  a 
second  impulse.  This  period  was  designated  b\'  pre- 
vious workers  as  the  'aljsolutely  refractory  period',  but 
more  recently  the  term  the  'least  (or  critical)  interval' 
between  two  effective  stimuli  (124,  136)  is  preferred. 
The  reason  for  this  recommendation  is  the  fact  that, 
when  one  determines  the  recovery  curve  at  the  site  of 
stimulation,  a  continuous  curve  without  a  break  is 
obtained.  The  term  'functional'  absolutely  refractory 
period  has  also  been  recommended  to  describe  this 
period  (103). 

The  period  during  which  the  excitability  recovers 
continuously  is  called  the  'relatively  refractory  period'. 
Following  this  period  there  is  often  a  period  of 
heightened  excitability  which  is  called  the  supernor- 
mal phase.  During  the  'supernormal  phase',  the  size  of 
the  action  potential  and  the  conduction  velocity  are 
practically  normal. 

The  thin  line  in  figure  4  shows  the  recovery  curve 
for  the  same  fiber  determined  at  low  temperature. 
The  temperature-dependence  of  the  recoxery  curve 
is  pronounced,  the  Qio  being  about  3.5  (2,  1 19).  The 
effect  of  temperature  change  is  reversible. 

The  conduction  velocity  is  known  to  be  subnormal 
during  the  relatively  refractory  period.  This  is  shown 
in  figure  5,  in  which  the  shock  response  intervals  for 
two  impulses  were  plotted  against  the  distance  be- 
tween the  site  of  stimulation  and  the  site  of  recording. 
The  two  impulses  were  set  up  at  an  interval  slightly 
longer  than  the  least  interval  of  the  fiber.  It  is  seen 
in  the  figure  that  the  shock  response  interval  for  the 
first  impulse  increases  proportionately  with  the  con- 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


10  15  20 

SHOCK    INTERVAL 


2  3  4  5  6 

SHOCK  RESPONSE    INTERVAL 


FIG.  4.  Recovery  curves  of  a  toad  nerse  hber  determined  at  two  different  temperatures.  [From 
Tasaki  (119).] 

FIG.  5.  Relation  between  the  conduction  distance  and  the  shock  response  interval  for  two  impulses 
elicited  at  an  interval  of  2  msec.  A  motor  nerve  fiber  of  1 1  /j  in  diameter  inner\.ating  the  flexor 
digitorum  brevis  of  the  toad.  Temperature,  23°C. 


duction  distance.  Evidently,  the  first  impulse  travels 
along  the  filler  at  a  normal  constant  rate. 

If  the  second  impulse  had  travelled  at  the  normal 
velocity,  the  shock  response  interval  for  the  second 
impulse  should  be  represented  by  the  dotted  line  in 
the  figure  which  has  the  same  slope  as  the  straight 
line  for  the  first  impulse.  Actually,  it  is  seen  that  the 
observed  shock  response  interval  increases  with  in- 
creasing conduction  distance  more  rapidly  than  that 
for  the  first  impulse. 

It  is  easy  to  figure  out  the  space-time  pattern  of  the 
two  impulses  based  on  the  experimental  data  present 
in  figure  5.  Evidently,  the  tangent  (slope)  of  the  curve 
in  the  figure  represents  the  velocity  of  the  second  im- 
pulse at  that  moment.  At  the  point  where  the  two 
impulses  were  initiated,  the  velocity  of  the  second 
impulse  is  approximately  50  per  cent  of  the  velocity 
of  the  first  impulse.  Because  of  this  large  difference  in 
velocity  between  the  two  impulses,  the  second  im- 
pulse lags,  spatially  and  temporally,  behind  the  first 
as  they  travel  along  the  fiber.  As  separation  between 
the  two  impulses  increases,  however,  the  second  im- 
pulse gains  more  speed  because  of  increasing  recovery 
from  the  refractoriness  left  behind  the  first  impulse. 
Thus,  as  they  travel  along  a  nerve  fiber,  the  interval 
between  the  two  impulses  approaches  asymptotically 
a  constant  value  which  is  independent  of  the  initial 
interval  at  which  they  started. 

c)  Two-way  conduction.  It  is  simple  to  demon- 
strate that  a  nerve  fiber  is  capable  of  carrying  im- 
pulses in  both  directions,  from  its  proximal  end 
toward  the  distal  and  also  in  the  reverse  direction. 
An  observation  illustrated  by  figure  6  shows  this. 
Here  a  squid  giant  axon  is  used.  An  entirely  analo- 


gous observation  has  been  made  on  the  vertebrate 
myelinated  ner\'e  fiber. 

The  axon  is  placed  in  a  pool  of  fresh  sea  water  on 
a  glass  plate.  Near  each  of  the  two  ends  of  the  axon 
a  pair  of  stimulating  electrodes  is  placed.  A  recording 
electrode,  a  glass  pipette  of  about  i  ix  at  the  tip  filled 
with  isosmotic  potassium  chloride  solution  in  this 
case,  is  pushed  into  the  axoplasm  of  the  axon  through 
its  .surface  membrane.  The  grounded  sea  water  is 
taken  as  a  reference  point  for  measuring  the  action 
potential.  A  stimulus  applied  at  one  end,  A  in  the 
figure,  gives  rise  to  a  response  of  the  all-or-none  type, 
indicating  that  the  impulse  starting  at  A  trav"els 
toward  B.  When  another  stimulating  shock  is  applied 
at  the  other  end,  B,  sometime  after  the  impulse  from 
A  has  swept  o\-er  the  fiber,  the  impulse  arising  at  B 
can  be  recorded  by  the  pipette  in  the  middle  of  the 
axon  (see  the  top  record  in  fig.  6 ).  Since  the  recording 
pipette  can  be  placed  anywhere  between  A  and  B 
with  essentially  the  same  result,  this  observation  proves 
that  the  axon  is  capable  of  carrying  impulses  in  both 
directions. 

When  the  time  intersal  between  the  shocks  at  A 
and  B  is  reduced  below  a  certain  limit  (see  the  record 
in  the  middle),  the  second  shock  becomes  ineffective. 
The  explanation  of  this  fact  is  simple.  Soon  after 
region  B  of  the  axon  is  traversed  by  the  impulse 
arising  at  A,  this  region  becomes  refractorv  and  does 
not  respond  to  the  second  shock. 

What  happens  if  two  shocks  are  applied  simulta- 
neously at  the  two  ends  A  and  B?  There  is  no  refrac- 
toriness at  the  site  of  stimulation  in  this  case  since 
these  regions  have  not  been  traversed  by  any  impulse. 
Hence,  an  impulse  should  be  initiated  at  A  propa- 


82 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


gating  toward  B.  Simultaneously,  another  impulse 
starting  at  B  should  travel  toward  A.  Then,  the  im- 
pulses are  bound  to  undergo  a  collision  at  a  point 
about  half  way  between  the  two  sites  of  stimulation 
of  the  axon.  After  such  a  head-on  collision,  it  should 
be  impossible  for  the  two  impulses  to  travel  further 
since  the  region  where  the  impulse  from  A  is  heading 
is  freshly  traversed  by  the  impulse  from  B  and  is  conse- 
quently incapable  of  carrying  another  impulse.  The 
same  thing  can  be  said  of  the  region  on  the  other  side 
of  the  site  of  collision. 

In  the  bottom  record  of  figure  6,  the  two  stimu- 
lating shocks  are  delivered  in  such  a  way  that  the  two 
impulses  collide  exactly  at  the  site  of  recording.  This 
is  accomplished  by  adjusting  the  delays  of  the  two 
shocks  after  the  start  of  the  sweep  of  the  oscillograph 
beam  in  such  a  manner  that  the  respon.se  to  shock  A 
alone  appears  at  the  same  spot  on  the  oscillograph 
screen  as  the  response  to  shock  B  alone.  Delivery  of 
two  shocks  under  these  conditions  elicits,  as  can  be 
seen  in  the  figure,  only  one  response  which  has 
almost  the  same  configuration  as  the  respon.se  to  one 
shock.  The  shock  response  interval  is  known  to  be 
slightly  reduced  by  collision.  A  further  discussion  on 
this  topic  may  be  found  elsewhere  (120). 

d)  Multiplication  of  impulses  at  the  branching  point  of  a 
nerve  fiber.  Histological  studies  indicate  that  vertebrate 
motor  nerve  fibers  frequently  undergo  dichotomy  or 
ramification  at  nodes  of  Ranvier,  one  mother  fiber 
giving  rise  to  two  (or  more)  daughter  fibers  [cf.  e.g. 
Eccles  &  Sherrington  (26)].  During  the  course 
of  isolating  single  nerve  fibers  innervating  the  toad 
gastrocnemius  muscle,  such  branching  motor  fibers 
are  sometimes  encountered.  It  has  been  shown 
in  such  preparations  that  the  muscle  tension  developed 
by  stimulation  of  the  mother  fiber  (with  two  daughter 
fibers  intact)  is  nearly  twice  as  great  as  the  tension 
observed  after  severing  one  of  the  daughter  fibers. 
Obviously,  this  indicates  that  the  iinpulse  travelling 
down  the  mother  fiber  invades  the  two  daughter 
fibers.  By  this  process  of  successive  dichotomy,  an 
impulse  travelling  along  a  motor  nerve  fiber  multi- 
plies itself  before  it  reaches  a  large  number  of  muscle 
fibers. 

Sensory  nerve  fibers  generally  dichotomize  as  they 
approach  their  peripheral  endings.  They  also  branch 
off  many  collaterals  in  the  spinal  cord.  It  is  generally 
believed  that  impulses  multiply  themselves  at  the.se 
bifurcating  points.  In  the  squid  axons,  multiplication 
of  impulses  at  bifurcation  points  has  also  been  ob- 
served. 

e)  Interaction    between    nerve  fibers.    When    a    group 


of  fibers  in  a  nerve  trunk  carries  nerve  impulses,  it 
never  happens,  under  ordinary  experimental  condi- 
tions, that  these  impulses  are  transmitted  to  the  other 
surrounding  nerve  fibers.  This  can  be  shown  bv  the 
following  simple  observation. 

The  gastrocnemius  muscle  of  the  toad  or  frog  is 
innervated  by  a  small  nerve  twig  branching  off  from 
the  large  tibial  nerve  which  innervates  also  plantar 
muscles  and  the  skin  of  the  foot.  Stimulation  of  the 
tibial  nerve  at  a  point  distal  to  the  exit  of  the  muscle 
nerve  to  the  gastrocnemius  does  not  evoke  any  po- 
tential variation  in  the  muscle  nerve  nor  any  contrac- 
tion in  the  muscle.  Such  a  stimulus  sets  up  a  '  volley 
of  impulses'  in  the  majority  of  the  fibers  in  the  tibial 
nerve,  but  these  impulses  do  not  spread  to  the  nerve 
fibers  entering  the  muscle. 

It  has  been  found,  however,  that  there  is  a  very 
weak,  barely  detectable  interaction  between  the 
nerve  fibers  in  a  common  nerve  trunk.  Otani  (96) 
found  that,  when  the  peroneal  branch  of  the  sciatic 
nerve  carries  a  volley  of  impulses,  the  threshold  for 
the  fibers  from  the  tibial  branch  undergoes  a  transient 
change.  This  observation  was  confirmed  and  ex- 
panded by  several  investigators,  notably  by  Marrazzi 
&  Lorente  de  No  (85).  This  result  is  now  interpreted 
on  a  purely  electrical  basis:  when  a  group  of  fibers 
carries  impulses,  the  fluid  in  the  intercellular  space  is 
traversed  by  action  currents  developed  by  these  active 
fibers.  If  a  stimulating  current  pulse  is  delivered  in 
this  region  of  nerve,  the  effect  of  the  stimulus  is 
modified  when  it  is  superposed  on  or  antagonized  by 
the  action  currents.  The  maximum  change  in  thresh- 
old is  of  the  order  of  10  per  cent. 

If  the  mechanism  of  interaction  between  nerve 
fibers  is  electrical  in  nature,  it  would  be  expected  that 
the  interaction  should  be  greatly  enhanced  by  re- 
ducing the  shunting  effect  of  the  fluid  medium  around 
the  nerve  fiber.  Katz  &  Schmitt  (73)  have  shown 
that  this  is  actually  the  case. 

The  diagram  at  the  top  of  figure  7  illustrates  their 
experimental  arrangement.  Two  nerve  fibers  of  the 
crab  were  immersed  in  a  pool  of  mineral  oil.  Fiber  I 
was  stimulated  with  electrodes  A  and  B  and  its  re- 
sponse was  observed  by  means  of  the  recording  elec- 
trodes D  and  E  in  the  figure.  At  about  the  time  of 
arrival  of  an  impulse  from  B  at  the  site  of  recording, 
testing  current  pulses  were  delivered  through  elec- 
trodes C  and  D  to  determine  changes  in  threshold  of 
fiber  II  at  D.  The  triphasic  curve  at  the  bottom  of 
figure  7  is  the  time  course  of  the  threshold  changes 
observed.  Katz  &  Schmitt  explained  these  results, 
with  good  reason,  as  due  to  the  flow  of  the  action  cur- 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


83 


^         I//  h'^    ^ 


FIG.  6.  Action  potentials  of  a  squid  giant  axon  elicited  by 
stimulating  shocks  at  the  two  ends,  A  and  B,  of  the  axon.  The 
recording  micropipctte  was  pushed  into  the  axoplasm  through 
the  axon  membrane.  Demonstration  of  two-way  conduction 
{top'),  refractoriness  (jniddli)  and  collision  of  impulses  {hol- 
torri).  Temperature,  '2o°C.  (Discussion  in  text.) 


rent  developed  by  fiber  I  through  the  surface  mem- 
brane of  fiber  II.  They  also  demonstrated  that  the 
velocity  of  an  impulse  in  fiber  II  is  afTected  by  the 
impulse  in  fiber  I  when  the  amount  of  the  fluid  is 
reduced  and  when  the  two  impulses  are  not  spatially 
far  apart. 

Arvanitaki  (9)  and  Tasaki  (124)  showed  that, 
under  special  experimental  conditions,  it  is  possible 
to  make  an  impulse  jump  from  one  fiber  to  another 
by  leading;  the  action  current  of  one  fiber  through  the 
other. 


CABLE    PROPERTIES    OF    THE    INVERTEBR.\TE    .^XON 

It  is  easy  to  introduce  a  small  glass  pipette  or  a  set 
of  metal  wires  longitudinally  into  a  squid  giant  axon. 
By  using  such  internal  electrodes,  electric  properties 
of  the  giant  axon  have  been  extensively  investigated 


by  a  number  of  physiologists.  We  shall  discuss  in  this 
section  some  of  the  basic  observations  which  serve  to 
clarify  electric  properties  of  the  resting  giant  axon 
(fig.  8). 

When  a  glass  pipette  electrode  of  about  100  \l  in 
diameter  is  inserted  longitudinally  into  a  giant  axon, 
it  is  found  that  the  potential  of  this  electrode  (relative 
to  the  large  ground  electrode  in  the  surrounding  sea 
water)  goes  down  gradually  as  the  pipette  electrode 
is  advanced  along  the  axis  of  the  axon.  The  potential 
inside  the  axon  is  negative  to  (i.e.  lower  than)  that 
of  the  surrounding  fluid  medium.  When  the  electrode 
is  advanced  more  than  about  10  mm  from  the  point 
of  insertion  on  the  surface  membrane,  the  potential 
level  of  the  axoplasm  is  practically  independent  of 
the  position  of  the  tip  of  the  pipette.  In  other  words, 
the  space  occupied  by  the  axoplasm  is  practically 
equipotential.  The  potential  difference  between  the 
surrounding  fluid  medium  and  the  axoplasm  deter- 
mined by  this  or  other  .similar  methods  is  called  the 
'resting  memljrane  potential'. 


I2(H 


Exciubility  change  in  fibre  [I 


T 


T 


FIG.  7.  Top:  Electrode  arrangement  used  for  demonstration 
of  excitability  changes  in  a  single  nerve  fiber  of  the  crab  caused 
by  the  passage  of  an  impulse  in  the  adjacent  fiber.  A,  B,  leads 
for  stimulation  of  fiber  I;  C,  D,  leads  for  stimulation  of  fiber  II; 
D,  E,  recording  leads  connecting  with  amplifier  and  cathode 
ray  oscillograph.  Bollom:  Excitability  changes  in  fiber  II  during 
the  passage  of  an  impulse  in  fiber  I.  Abscissae:  time  in  msec. 
Ordinates:  threshold  intensity  of  fiber  II  in  percentage  of  its 
resting  threshold.  [From  Katz  &  .Schmitt  (73).] 


84 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


I  y///  ////////  //T 


FIG.  8.  .1.  Resting  and  action  potential  of  the  squid  giant  axon  recorded  witli  an  intracellular  glass 
pipette  electrode.  Time  marker  (0.5  msec.)  indicates  the  potential  level  observed  when  the  recording 
electrode  was  in  the  surrounding  sea  water.  Temperature,  23°C.  B.  Exponential  variation  in  the 
meiBbrane  potential  caused  by  passage  of  a  constant  current  through  the  membrane  of  a  squid  giant 
axon  with  a  long  intracellular  silver  wire  electrode.  The  thick  portions  of  the  wire  in  the  diagram 
on  the  top  show  the  exposed  surface  of  the  electrodes.  Time  marker,  1000  cycles  per  sec.  Temperature, 
20°C.  (The  axons  in  the  diagrams  are  disproportionately  thick  and  short.) 


The  fact  that  the  potential  level  is  the  same  every- 
where in  the  axoplasm  indicates,  according  to  Ohm's 
law,  that  there  is  no  measurealjle  flow  of  electric  cur- 
rent in  the  axoplasm  at  rest.  It  also  proves  that  the 
resting  potential  represents,  as  in  the  frog  muscle 
fiber  (76)  and  in  other  nervous  elements,  a  sharp  drop 
of  electric  potential  across  the  space  occupied  by  the 
thin  surface  memijrane  of  the  cell.  The  resting  poten- 
tial of  an  excised  squid  giant  axon  is  known  to  be  50 
to  60  mv;  it  is  considerably  smaller  than  that  of  verte- 
brate skeletal  muscle  and  nerve  cells. 

When  a  pulse  of  stimulating  current  is  applied  to 
a  giant  axon  with  an  internal  recording  electrode, 
there  occurs  a  transient  rise  of  100  to  120  mv  in  the 
potential  of  the  axoplasm  referred  to  ground  (fig.  8.-1). 
The  magnitude  of  the  action  potential  measured  by 
this  method  is  practically  independent  of  the  position 
of  the  electrode  tip  in  the  axoplasm.  If  the  tip  of  the 
internal  electrode  touches  or  pierces  the  surface  mem- 
brane, both  the  resting  and  action  potentials  are  pro- 
foundly diminished  or  completeh'  eliminated.  The 
action  potential  represents,  therefore,  a  transient 
variation  of  the  potential  difference  across  the  surface 


membrane  of  the  axon.  It  is  important  to  distinguish 
this  'memljrane  action  potential'  from  those  recorded 
with  external  electrodes. 

When  it  was  discovered  that  the  membrane  action 
potential  is  suisstantially  larger  than  the  resting  po- 
tential of  the  membrane  (22,  56),  some  investigators 
who  believed  the  membrane  hypothesis  of  Bernstein 
(10)  were  greatly  surprised.  In  1902  Bernstein  postu- 
lated, without  clear  supporting  e\-idcnce,  that  the 
action  potential  may  be  a  mere  diminution  or  disap- 
pearance of  the  resting  memijrane  potential  (see 
p.  117).  The  finding  that  the  inside  potential  rises 
above  the  outside  potential  near  the  peak  of  the  ac- 
tion potential,  therefore,  conflicts  with  this  postulate 
of  the  membrane  hypothesis. 

Besides  the  role  in  maintaining  a  potential  differ- 
ence, the  surface  membrane  of  the  resting  axon  plays 
another  important  part  in  electrophysiology  of  the 
nerve  fiber.  The  resting  membrane  has  a  high  re- 
sistance to  a  direct  current.  This  can  be  shown  by 
the  use  of  the  arrangement  of  figure  Bfi,  in  which  a  set 
of  two  metal  wire  electrodes  was  used  instead  of  a 
glass  pipette. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


85 


The  electrode  set  shown  in  the  figure  consists  of  one 
wire  with  a  long  (about  1 2  mm)  exposed  surface  and 
the  other  with  a  short  (i  mm)  exposed  surface.  The 
long  wire  is  used  to  send  a  constant  current  into  the 
axon  and  the  other  for  recording  potential  changes 
caused  by  the  current.  The  short  electrode  has  its 
exposed  (uninsulated)  surface  in  the  middle  of  the 
long  one.  The  remaining  surface  of  each  electrode  is 
insulated  with  a  layer  of  enamel.  A  pulse  of  constant 
current  can  be  generated  by  connecting  a  high 
voltage  source  to  the  current  electrode  through  a 
high  resistance. 

When  the  sign  of  the  applied  current  is  such  that 
the  axon  membrane  is  traversed  by  an  inward  di- 
rected current,  the  potential  inside  the  membrane  is 
found  to  be  lowered  by  the  current.  However,  as  can 
be  seen  in  the  record  in  the  figure,  the  potential 
change  at  the  onset  of  the  current  is  gradual — 
mathematically  speaking,  exponential.  The  potential 
change  varies  roughly  proportionately  with  the  in- 
tensity of  the  applied  current.  When  the  current  is 
reversed,  the  sign  of  the  potential  change  is  simply 
reversed,  provided  that  the  change  in  the  resting 
potential  does  not  exceed  about  5  mv. 

This  behavior  of  the  axon  can  be  readily  under- 
stood if  one  assumes  that  the  axon  membrane  con- 
sists of  a  condenser  with  a  parallel  resistance  (fig.  gA). 
As  is  well  known,  the  current  flowing  through  a  con- 
denser of  a  capacity,  C,  is  given  by  C  dV/dt,  where 
dV/dt  is  the  rate  of  change  in  the  potential  difference, 
V,  across  the  condenser.  The  current,  /,  through  a 
system   of  a  conden.ser  and   a   parallel   resistance   is 


given  by  the  expression 


A 


B 


il 


out 


T 


[)  i  (!)  in  "5  ~ 

r,flx      V(x-4X,t)       V(x,t)       y(x+ftx,t) 


dV       V 

I  =  C 1-  - , 

dt        R 


(4-0 


i.e.  by  the  sum  of  the  capacitati\'e  current  and  the 
ohmic  current.  When  the  current,  the  capacity  and 
the  resistance,  /?,  are  all  constant,  the  time  course  of 
the  potential  is  given  by 


IR  (i   -  e-"«'0, 


(4-0 


Cm-AX         rm/4X 


v  =  o 


where  I  is  the  time  after  the  onset  of  the  current.  By 
comparing  equation  (4-2)  with  the  observed  result  of 
figure  8B,  the  values  of  R  and  C  can  be  determined. 
The  capacity,  C,  of  the  giant  axon  membrane  deter- 
mined by  this  method  is  approximately  i  /xf/cm- 
and  the  membrane  resistance  is  between  i  and 
2.5  kl2-cm-.  [cf.  Hodgkin  et  al.  (61),  p.  440].  The 
time  constant  of  the  membrane,  RC,  is,  therefore, 
I   to  2.5  msec.-' 

In  the  argument  developed  abo\e,  the  resistances 
of  both  the  axoplasm  and  the  sea  water  have  been 
ignored.  Cole  &  Hodgkin  (20)  and  Schmitt  (ro6) 
have  shown  that  the  axoplasm  is  a  homogeneous  con- 
ductor with  a  specific  resistance  of  about  40  ohm -cm 
at  2o°C.  The  specific  resistance  of  the  sea  water  is 
approximately  20  ohm -cm  at  the  same  temperature. 
These  resistances  are  too  small  to  have  any  observable 
effect  upon  the  measurement  of  figure  QB. 

Now  the  cjuestion  arises  of  how  the  voltage  .source 
representing  the  resting  membrane  potential  fits  in 
the  system  of  a  capacity  and  a  parallel  resistance  of 
figure  9.-I.  It  is  po.ssible  to  draw  a  continuous  current 
from  the  resting  membrane;  therefore,  it  is  legitimate 
lo  represent  the  source  of  the  resting  potential  by  a 
battery.  There  are  obviously  two  simple  wa\s,  B  and 
C  in  the  figure,  of  connecting  a  battery  in  the  circuit 
ot  A.  Both  ways  fit  with  the  obser\ed  data.  There  is 
at  present  no  direct  experimental  procedure  that  can 
serve  to  determine  which  one  of  them  represents  the 
axon  membrane  better.  In  the  sodium  theory  (cf 
p.  118),  the  electromotive  force  of  the  membrane  is 
assumed  to  be  connected  in  parallel  with  the  con- 
denser as  in  B. 

As  the  result  of  the  above  discussion,  it  has  become 
clear  that  a  squid  axon  behaves  like  the  core-conduc- 
tor of  Hermann  (see  p.  75)  or  like  a  submarine  cable. 
Using  elementary  calculus,  we  may  proceed  slightly 


FIG.  9.  Structure  of  the  squid  giant  axon  revealed  by  the 
use  of  intracellular  electrodes.  C,  capacity,  and  R,  resistance 
of  the  membrane.  Two  possible  ways  of  connecting  the  source 
of  the  resting  potential  in  the  circuit  of  R  and  C  arc  shown  by 
diagrams  B  and  C.  (Further  detail  in  te.xt.) 


^  These  figures  were  obtained  by  eliminating  the  effect  of 
the  current  flowing  near  the  end  of  the  current  electrode  by 
the  technique  described  by  Marmont  (84).  The  reader  is 
reminded  in  this  connection  to  pay  attention  also  to  the  di- 
mensions of  these  figures. 


86 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


further   to  discuss  the  spread   of  electricity  alons  a 
uniform  resting  axon. 

In  figure  qD,  the  electric  properties  of  an  axon  im- 
mersed in  a  large  volume  of  sea  water  are  represented 
by  a  network  of  resistances  and  capacities.  Since  we 
are  interested  only  in  the  change  of  potentials,  the 
batteries  are  omitted  in  the  figure.  The  resistance  of 
the  axoplasm  of  a  unit  length  is  represented  by  r-,;  it 
is  related  to  the  specific  resistance  of  the  axoplasm 
Ri  by  the  expression 


"■i  = 


(4-3) 


where  D  is  the  diameter  of  the  fiber. 

Symbols  fn,  and  („,  denote,  respectively,  the  resist- 
ance and  the  capacity  of  the  memljrane  covering  the 
axoplasm  of  a  unit  length.  They  are  related  to  the 
corresponding  figures  for  a  unit  area,  R„.  and  Cm,  by 
the  formulae 

':«    =    -^  .  (4-4) 

c„,   =   ttDC,,,.  (4-5) 

Let  V(^x,  t)  denote  the  potential  of  the  axoplasm, 
referred  to  the  potential  of  the  surrounding  fluid 
medium,  at  position  x  and  time  /.  Then  the  ssmi^ols 
f'(.v  —  A.v,  0  and  ['(v  +  ^x,  0  can  be  used  to  denote 
the  potentials  at  position  (,v  —  A.v)  and  at  (.v  -|-  A.v), 
respectively.  The  axon  is  now  imaginati\elv  divided 
into  a  series  of  .segments  of  a  length  lA.v.  The  a.xoplasm 
resistance  (to  a  longitudinal  current)  of  such  a  seg- 
ment is  then  rjA.v.  Similarly,  the  membrane  capacity 
and  the  resistance  of  one  segment  are  given  by 
c„Ax  and  rn,/A,v,  respectively.  By  applying  Ohm's 
law,  it  is  found  that  the  longitudinal  current  in  the 
section  labelled  i  is  equal  to  [("(.v,  /)  —  '(•*  —  ^x,  0]/ 
(riAAr).  Similarly,  the  longitudinal  current  through 
■section  2  is  equal  to  [F(.v  +  A.v,  /)  —  r'(.v,  0]/C''iA.v). 
The  difference  between  the  current  through  i  and 
that  through  2  is  equal  to  the  membrane  current, 
which  has  the  form  given  by  equation  (4-1).  This 
us  to  the  equation 

I^Cv  +  \x.  0  -   r(.v,  /)        F(.v,  0  -  F(.v  -  Sx.O 


r,Ax 


r\Ax 

dVix,  0        Vix,  0 

=  f„,A.v 1 . 

dl  r,^/Ax 


By  taking  the  limit  A.v  to  zcio,  we  obtain  the  well 
known  cable  equation: 


r,  a.v2 


dl'Cx,  0 


It  is  obvious  that  the  spread  of  currents  in  other  non- 
myelinated nerve  fibers  and  in  a  uniform  muscle  fiber 
can  be  described  by  the  same  equation. 

In  the  steady  state  the  potential  is  a  function  of 
position  X  alone.  Equation  (4-6)  is  then  reduced  to 


dnXx) 

dx^ 


=  VixX 


(4-7) 


in  which  \\x)  represents  ['(a:,  k).  The  general  solu- 
tion of  this  equation  is 


r(.v)  =  Aft-^l^-  -|-Be+''\ 


C4-8) 


where  X,  the  'space  constant',  is  related  to  the  mem- 
brane resistance  and  the  axoplasm  resistance  by  the 
expression 

Constants  A  and  B  in  equation  (4-8)  depend  on  the 
boundary  conditions. 

In  a  special  case  where  a  constant  current  of  in- 
tensity /o  is  .sent  into  the  axon  at  .v  =  o,  constant  B 
has  to  be  equal  to  zero;  otherwise,  \\x')  approaches 
infinity  as  .v  increases.  At  .v  =  o  where  the  current  is 
sent  into  the  axon,  dr(A)'d.v  is  equal  to  —  '2  ''i^»  >  the 
factor  '2  being  introduced  to  meet  the  situation  where 
the  current  spreads  on  both  sides  of  the  point  .v  =  o. 
From  these  boundary  conditions,  it  is  found  that 
A  =  ^-n^lfi  and  B  =  o.  The  solution  of  equation 
(4-7)  for  this  special  case  is,  therefore. 


K*)  =  }i  nX-Zoe- 


C4-10) 


at 


-h  Vix,  0. 


(4-6) 


The  'effective'  resistance  }-2^i^  can  be  expressed  by 
virtue  of  equation  (4-9)  as  3^2  ^/^^n^i  ■  The  space 
constant,  X,  is  a  measure  of  the  spread  of  electricity 
along  the  axon;  the  greater  the  value  of  X,  the  more 
extensive  is  the  spread.  In  the  squid  giant  axon,  X  is 
of  the  order  of  0.6  cm  (20).  Solutions  of  the  general 
cable  equation  for  several  special  cases  have  been 
achieved  (30,  63,  130). 


C'SiBLE    PROPERTIES   OF   THE    MYELINATED    NERVE    FIBER 

Large  nerve  fibers  in  the  vertebrate  nerve  have  a 
thick  layer  of  fatty  substance,  the  myelin  sheath,  be- 
tween the  cylinder  of  the  axoplasm  and  the  outermost 
layer  of  connective  tissue,  the  neurilemma  or  the 
sheath  of  Schwann.  The  myelin  sheath  is  broken  at 
so-called  nodes  of  Ranvier  where  the  surface  of  the 
axis  cvlindcr  is  covered  dircctlv  bv  the  neurilemma. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


87 


The  width  of  the  nodal  membrane  uncovered  by  the 
myehn  sheath  is  roughly  0.5  to  i  m-  The  distance  be- 
tween the  nodes  has  been  discussed  on  p.  78. 

The  first  experimental  evidence  indicating  that  the 
myelin  sheath  has  a  high  resistance  to  a  direct  current 
was  obtained  in  Tokyo  in  1934  [Kubo,  Ono  &  Tasaki 
cited  in  (70)].  When  the  threshold  of  an  isolated 
single  nerve  fiber  was  determined  with  a  small 
electrode  placed  near  the  fiber,  it  was  found  that  the 
threshold  varied  regularly  with  the  distance  from  the 
nodes  of  the  fiber  (fig.  10).  In  these  early  experiments 
the  threshold  was  determined  by  taking  twitches  of 
the  muscle  innervated  by  the  isolated  fiber  as  an  index 
that  a  nerve  impulse  had  been  initiated  in  the  fiber. 
Later,  measurements  were  made  by  taking  electric 
responses  of  the  fiber  as  an  index  [e.g.  fig.  i  in  Tasaki 
(123)].  All  these  experiments  indicate  that  the 
threshold  is  lowest  when  the  small  electrode  (stimu- 
lating cathode)  is  placed  directly  on  one  of  the  nodes 
(the  other  electrode  placed  in  the  fluid  medium  away 
from  the  fiber),  and  is  highest  when  the  electrode  is  at 
the  point  half  way  between  two  neighboring  nodes. 
These  findings  have  been  interpreted  as  indicating 
that,  because  of  the  high  (d.c.)  resistance  of  the 
myelin  sheath,  the  stimulating  current  enters  and 
leaves  only  at  the  nodes  and  consequently  that  the 
nerve  fiber  is  excited  only  at  the  nodes.  A  further 
discussion  on  this  subject  may  be  found  elsewhere 
(71,    124). 

It  was  found  later  that  the  myelin  sheath  is  not  a 
perfect  insulator  but  that  short  current  pulses  can  flow 


readily  through  this  sheath  (66,  1:24,  125,  136).  To 
illustrate  this  point,  we  shall  mention  an  observation 
published  in  Germany  during  World  War  II  (136). 
The  diagram  in  figure  11. 4  illustrates  the  experi- 
mental arrangement  used. 

A  single  nerve  fiber  of  the  toad  is  mounted  across 
three  small  pools  of  Ringer's  solution  divided  by  two 
narrow  air  gaps  of  o.i  to  0.3  mm  width.  The  pool  in 
the  middle  is  about  i  mm  wide  and  contains  only  the 
myelin  covered  part  of  the  fiber.  All  the  nodes  (Ni, 
N2  and  others)  are  kept  in  the  larger,  lateral  pools.  In 


\ 


\/ 


FIG.  10.  Threshold  strength  of  a  long  stimulating  current 
(in  amperes)  plotted  against  distance  from  a  node  of  Ranvier, 
Ni.  Motor  nerve  fiber  of  the  toad  immersed  in  a  shallow  pool 
of  Ringer.  Black  circles  show  the  results  obtained  with  the 
cathode  of  the  battery  connected  to  the  microelectrode,  and 
the  crosses  with  current  flowing  in  the  opposite  direction. 
Temperature,  23°C.  [From  Tasaki  (124).] 


_n_ 


-T 


No  N,  N2 


1 

WHtt/t 

t 1    msec 

FIG.  II.  .-i.  Membrane  current  led  through  i  mm  long  myelin  covered  portion  of  toad  motor  nerve 
fiber.  B.  Similar  to  .1 ;  there  is  a  node  (Ni)  in  the  middle  pool.  The  fibers  were  stimulated  through 
the  electrode  on  the  nerve  trunk.  Note  that  the  action  potential  at  the  node  is  about  0.9  msec,  dura- 
tion at  24°C.  (The  nerve  fiber  in  the  diagram  is  disproportionately  thick  and  short.) 


88 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH%SIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


each  of  the  pools,  a  nonpolarizable  electrode  is  im- 
mersed. The  electrodes  in  the  lateral  pools  are  directly 
grounded  and  the  one  in  the  middle  pool  is  grounded 
through  a  resistor  of  o.  i  to  0.3  megohms.  The  cur- 
rents produced  by  the  fiber  in  response  to  an  electric 
shock  applied  to  the  fiber  near  its  cut  end  are  recorded 
by  amplifying  the  IR  drop  across  the  resistor. 

If  the  myelin  sheath  were  a  perfect  insulator  of 
electricity,  no  flow  ol  current  should  be  recorded  with 
this  arrangement.  Actually,  a  relatively  strong  flow  of 
current  is  observed  through  the  myelin  sheath.  As  can 
be  seen  in  the  records  of  figure  11.^,  the  membrane 
current  led  through  the  myelin  sheath  has  clear 
double  peaks  of  an  outward  flow,  followed  by  a  long 
phase  of  a  weak  inward  current. 

When  a  node  of  Ranvier  is  introduced  into  the 
middle  pool  (fig.  iiE),  an  entirely  different  result  is 
obtained.  The  flow  of  current  through  the  membrane 
of  the  fiber  in  the  middle  pools  is  triphasic,  first  out- 
ward, then  inward  and  finally  outward  (weak). 
Comparing  the  two  records  in  figure  11,  it  is  found 
that  a  strong  flow  of  inward  current  takes  place  only 
at  the  nodes  of  Ranvier.  Since  the  total  amount  of 
current  leaving  a  fiber  at  any  moment  has  to  be  equal 
to  the  sum  of  the  current  entering  the  fiber  at  the 
same  moment,  the  peaks  of  the  outward  current 
through  the  myelin  sheath  (record  A)  should  corre- 
spond roughly  to  the  peaks  of  inward  current  at  the 
neighboring  nodes  (Ni  and  N2).  The  effects  of  more 
distant  nodes  are  naturally  far  smaller  than  those  of 
the  neighboring  nodes. 

That  the  first  peak  in  record  .-1  of  figure  1 1  is  caused 
by  the  response  at  node  Ni  and  the  second  peak  by 
the  response  at  N2  has  been  shown  in  the  following 
manner.  When  a  few  drops  of  cocaine-Ringer's  solu- 
tion are  introduced  in  the  lateral  pool  in  which  N2  is 
immersed,  the  height  of  the  second  peak  is  immedi- 
ately reduced.  When  the  same  cocaine-Ringer's  solu- 
tion is  applied  to  the  portion  of  the  nerve  fiber  in  the 
middle  pool,  no  change  in  the  current  is  observed. 
Finally,  v\hen  the  narcotizing  solution  is  introduced 
gradually  into  the  pool  of  Ni,  the  height  of  the  first 
peak  is  gradually  reduced,  while  the  second  peak  re- 
mains unchanged  until  it  disappears  suddenly  at  the 
moment  when  the  propagation  of  the  impulse  is 
blocked. 

Further  evidence  indicating  that  electric  responses  of 
a  myelinated  nerve  fiber  are  evocable  only  at  the 
nodes  of  Ranvier  has  been  obtained  by  narcotizing 
the  portions  of  the  fiber  located  in  the  lateral  pools 
and  stimulating  the  fiber  through  two  of  the  elec- 
trodes (124,    132).  When   there  is  one   node  in   the 


middle  pool  (as  in  the  diagram  of  fig.  11 B),  a  full- 
sized  action  current  can  be  recorded  from  a  short  (i 
mm)  nonnarcotized  portion  of  the  nerve  fiber.  But, 
when  no  node  is  left  in  the  normal  Ringer's  solution 
in  the  middle  pool  (as  in  fig.  i  lA),  no  action  current 
can  be  elicited  from  the  fiber. 

The  size  01  the  membrane  action  potential  at  the 
node  was  estimated  by  Tasaki  &  Takcuchi  (135)  by 
measuring  the  action  current  and  the  resistance  of 
the  single  fiber  preparation.  Huxley  &  Stampfli  (67) 
estimated  it  by  compensating  the  action  current  with 
an  external  voltage  source  (assuming  that  the  myelin 
sheath  is  a  perfect  insulator).  Later,  a  direct  method 
of  recording  the  action  potential  of  the  nodal  mem- 
brane was  developed  (128).  All  the.se  indirect  and 
direct  methods  give  a  figure  between  95  and  115  mv 
at  the  peak  of  activity.  Later,  we  shall  discuss  the 
difTerence  between  the  shape  of  the  nodal  action 
potential  and  that  of  the  squid  action  potential. 

If  one  assumes  that  the  rnyelin  sheath  behaves  like 
a  condenser  with  a  parallel  resistance  as  shown  by 
the  diagram  of  figure  g.^,  the  flow  of  current  through 
the  myelin  sheath  should  be  described  by  equation 
(4-1)  in  the  preceding  section.  The  voltage  I'  in  the 
equation  can  be  either  an  applied  voltage  or  an 
action  potential  developed  at  the  nodes.  The  two 
peaks  in  the  current  flowing  through  the  myelin 
sheath  (fig.  11. -1^,  therefore,  are  indicative  of  the 
situation  in  which  the  voltage  inside  the  myelin 
sheath  rises  in  two  steps,  one  step  at  the  beginning  of 
the  action  potential  at  Ni  and  the  other  step  when 
X>  is  also  activated.  Actually,  the  time  interval  be- 
tween the  two  peaks  is  close  to  the  internodal  con- 
duction time  discussed  previously  on  p.  79. 

It  requires  a  slight  mathematical  treatment  of  the 
data  to  separate  the  current  led  through  the  myelin 
sheath  into  its  capacitative  and  ohmic  components 
and  to  determine  the  absolute  values  for  the  capacity, 
(■„,,  and  the  resistance  ;„,,  of  the  myelin  sheath  (125). 
Although  this  method  of  measuring  the  membrane 
capacity  and  the  resistance  is  not  as  direct  as  that 
for  the  squid  axon,  the  accuracy  of  the  measurement 
is  fairly  high  (the  probable  error  being  about  10  per 
cent).  The  results  of  recent  measurements  of  these 
membrane  constants  are  listed  in  the  uppermost 
column  of  table  i.  The  observed  values  of  f,„  and  r,,. 
were  converted  into  the  values  for  myelin  sheath  of  a 
unit  area  (represented  by  capitalized  figures)  by  using 
equations  (4-4)  and  (4-5)  in  the  preceding  section. 

The  capacity  and  the  resistance  of  the  nodal  mem- 
brane given  in  table  i  were  determined  by  measuring 
the  current  through  node  (Ni)  in  the  middle  pool  of 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


89 


TABLE  I .  Resistances  and  Capacities  nf  the  Myelin  Sheath,  the  Squid  Axon  and  the  Nodal  Membrane 


farad/cm 


farad/cm* 


ohn 


ohm-cm^ 


Myelin  sheath  (fiber  diameter  12  fi) 
Squid  giant  axon  (diameter  500  ii) 
Nodal  membrane 


.6  X  lo-i 
.6  X  10-" 
I  -5  y-t^^ 


5  X  10-9 

ID"* 

(3-7)  X  lo-s 


2.9  X  10' 
(6-15)  X  10' 

41  Mn* 


10* 

(1-2.5)  X  io» 
8-20 


Data  from  references  (20,  61,  125). 

*  Values  for  one  whole  node  of  Ranvier  of  the  toad  motor  ner\e  fiber. 


figure  iii5,  alter  treatint;  this  node  with  a  sodium- 
free  Ringer's  solution  or  with  a  dilute  cocaine- 
Ringer's  solution.  The  details  of  the  principle  of  the 
method  can  be  found  elsewhere  (125).  Since  it  is 
difficult  to  estimate  the  area  of  the  nodal  membrane, 
the  figures  for  a  unit  area  of  the  nodal  memijrane  are 
somewhat  inaccurate.  For  comparison,  the  membrane 
constants  of  the  squid  giant  axon  are  also  listed  in  the 
same  table. 

It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the  capacity  of  ijoth 
the  myeHn  sheath  and  of  the  nodal  membrane  is  ex- 
tremely insensitive  to  changes  in  the  temperature  and 
the  chemical  composition  of  the  surrounding  fluid 
medium,  while  their  resistance  can  be  strongly  modi- 
fied by  slight  changes  in  the  environinent  C'-4>  125). 
There  is,  however,  one  siinple  way  of  increasing  the 
capacity  of  the  inyelin  sheath,  that  is,  by  dissolving 
the  fatty  substance  of  the  myelin  sheath  by  an  appli- 
cation of  a  saponin-Ringer's  solution  or  some  other 
detergent  solution.  During  the  early  stage  of  a 
saponin  treatment  of  the  myelin  sheath,  the  capacity 
increases  as  the  resistance  decreases,  the  product 
c,„r,„  remaining  almost  unchanged.  This  fact  strongly 
suggests  that  the  capacity  of  the  inyelin  sheath  is 
dielectric  in  nature,  determined  by  the  thickness  of 
the  sheath  and  the  dielectric  constant  of  the  myelin 
substance.  The  dielectric  constant  of  the  myelin  sheath 
is  known  to  be  similar  to  that  of  many  other  fatty 
compounds  (66,  125). 


CONDUCT.\NCE  OF  THE  MEMBR.ANE  DURING  .ACTIVITY 

VVe  have  seen  in  the  preceding  section  that  the 
development  of  the  action  potential  represents  a  tran- 
sient variation  in  the  potential  difference  across  the 
surface  membrane  of  the  nerve  fiber.  In  1939,  Cole  & 
Curtis  C19)  demonstrated  in  the  .squid  giant  axon 
that  this  variation  in  the  meinbrane  potential  is  asso- 
ciated with  a  pronounced  change  in  the  resistance  of 
the  membrane.  Tasaki  &  Mizuguchi  C'SS)  showed  a 
similar  change  in  the  membrane  at  the  node  of  Ran- 


\ier.  We  shall  discuss  the  principle  of  measuring  the 
membrane  impedance  during  activity  under  relatively 
simple  experimental  conditions.  The  method  to  he 
described  is  slightly  different  from  that  employed  by 
Cole  &  Courtis  but  the  principle  is  the  same. 

In  the  arrangement  shown  in  the  upper  part  of 
figure  12,  a  long  silver  wire  electrode  about  100  ^i  in 
diameter  is  thrust  into  a  squid  a.xon  immersed  in  sea 
water  This  internal  electrode  and  a  large  electrode 
immersed  in  sea  water  surrounding  the  a.xon  are 
connected    to    one    arm    of   an    alternatino    current 


FIG.  \i.  Measurement  of  the  membrane  impedance  of  a 
squid  giant  axon  during  activity  with  an  a.c.  impedance 
bridge.  The  bridge  was  balanced  for  the  impedance  of  the 
resting  membrane.  The  two  records  on  the  left  were  taken  at 
nearly  the  same  stimulus  intensity,  but  the  bridge  output  was 
amplified  10  times  the  normal  (ix)  in  the  lower  record.  The 
upper  trace  in  the  records  displays  the  unfiltered  bridge  out- 
put; the  potentials  recorded  are  slightly  reduced  and  distorted 
by  the  bridge.  The  bridge  a.c,  20  kc  per  sec;  temperature, 
22  °C.  (Further  discussion  in  text.} 


90 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Wheatstone  bridge.  The  ratio  arms,  ri  and  r-2  in  the 
figure,  consist  of  ohmic  resistors,  r2-ri  being  io:i  or 
larger.  The  remaining  arm  consists  of  condensers 
(C  and  C)  and  a  resistor  (/f).  When  a  high  fre- 
quency alternating  current  is  applied  to  the  bridge,  a 
sinusoidal  potential  variation  is  produced  across  the 
membrane.  By  proper  adjustment  of  the  variable 
resistance  and  the  capacity,  however,  it  is  po.ssible  to 
reduce  the  a.c.  output  of  the  bridge  to  zero. 

As  has  been  mentioned  above  (p.  85)  the  axon 
membrane  can  be  represented  by  a  condenser  and 
parallel  resistance.  The  relationship  between  the  po- 
tential difference  (F)  across  the  membrane  and  the 
current  (/)  through  the  membrane  is  expressed  by 
equation  (4-1)  which  can  be  rewritten  as 


Therefore, 


I  =  C \-  GV, 

di 


(6-1) 


where  G  is  the  conductance  of  the  membrane,  i.e.  the 
reciprocal  of  resistance  R  in  equation  (4-1)-  We  are 
now  interested  in  the  relation  between  a  steady 
sinu.soidal  current  and  a  sinusoidal  voltage  that  satis- 
fies equation  (6-1).  We  denote  the  current  by 


/ii  sin  ut 


and  the  voltage  bv 


V  =    I'll  sin  (ail  +  9), 


(6-2a) 


C6-2b) 


where  /o  and  i'l,  are  the  current  amplitude  and  the 
voltage  amplitude,  respectively,  co  is  27r  times  the 
frequency  and  9  the  phase  difference  between  the 
current  and  the  voltage.  Introducing  (6-2a  and  b) 
into  (6-1),  we  find  that 

/o  sin  u>t  =    I'd  o>C  cos  (u/  +  d}  +  GVo  sin  (_uit  +  S) 
=    Vq  (G  cos  6  —  o>C  sin  $')  sin  u/ 
+   Vo  (C  sin  e  +  uC  cos  9)  cos  oil 

The  last  equation  is  satisfied  when  (and  only  when) 
the  coefficient  of  cos  a;/  is  zero  and  simultaneously 
when  the  coeflicients  of  sin  oj/  on  both  sides  of  the 
equation  are  equal.  This  leads  to  the  relations 


(6-3a) 


and 

/u  =    Vd  (C  cos  e  —  uC  sin  9). 
From  equation  (6-3a)  it  follows  that 

— (jC  G 


h  =   t'o  y/o^C  -f-  G2 


Fo  =  /„ 


Vw^C'  -I-  (? ' 


(6-3b) 


y/ufC'^  -h  G2 ' 


V"'C2  -I-  G^ 


When  the  impedance  bridge  in  the  upper  part  of 
figure  12  is  roughly  balanced  for  a  given  intensity  of 
the  bridge  a.c,  the  current  /  through  the  axon  mem- 
brane is  determined  by  the  variable  condensers  and 
the  variable  resistance  of  the  bridge,  because  r-i  » 
ri.  Under  these  conditions,  the  amplitude  \\  is  pro- 
portional to  the  impedance,  i  /-\/G-  -|-  co^C',  of  the 
membrane.  When  G  increases  during  activity,  Fn  de- 
creases. In  the  method  involving  use  of  the  impedance 
bridge,  small  changes  in  the  membrane  impedance  are 
detected  by  balancing  the  bridge  with  the  membrane 
impedance  at  rest  and  recording  small  unbalances 
after  a  high  amplification.  Under  such  circum- 
stances, not  only  a  change  in  the  amplitude  \'n  but 
also  any  change  in  the  phase  d  brings  about  a  bridge 
unbalance.  When  the  bridge  is  at  balance,  the  voltage 
between  the  two  electrodes  across  the  axon  membrane 
is  completely  cancelled  by  the  voltage  across  ri.  A 
change  in  the  phase  6  or  in  the  amplitude  Fo  ,  makes 
this  cancellation  imperfect. 

[In  order  to  detect  changes  in  the  membrane  im- 
pedance during  activity,  it  is  necessary  to  make  the 
frequency  of  the  bridge  a.c.  high  enough  so  that  in 
the  period  to  be  examined  there  are  a  number  of  full 
cycles  of  the  a.c.  The  time  resolution  in  the  im- 
pedance measurement  is  affected  also  by  the  char- 
acteristic of  the  filter  circuit  in  the  recording  system.] 

.After  the  Wheatstone  bridge  has  been  accurately 
balanced  for  the  membrane  impedance  at  rest,  a  short 
pulse  of  outward  current  is  passed  through  the  axon 
membrane.  If  this  pulse  is  well  below  the  threshold, 
the  potential  trace  (the  upper  trace  in  the  records  of 
fig.  12)  shows  an  exponential  decay  of  the  membrane 
potential  after  the  end  of  the  pulse;  in  this  case  there 
is  very  little  or  no  bridge  unbalance  detectable.  When 
the  pulse  intensity  approaches  the  threshold,  the  fall 
of  the  membrane  potential  after  termination  of  the 
pulse  becomes  slow  and  erratic  (see  p.  98);  con- 
comitantly there  is  a  sign  of  a  decrease  in  the  mem- 
brane impedance  (record  .-1)  which  can  be  recorded 
distinctly  by  increasing  the  amplification  of  the  a.c. 
bridge  output  (record  B).  With  supra  threshold  pulse 
intensities,  large  unbalances  of  the  bridge  are  ob- 
served (record  C),  indicating  that  there  is  a  marked 


reduction  in  the  membrane  impedance  associated 
with  production  of  an  action  potential. 

The  temporal  relation  between  the  action  potential 
and  the  bridge  unbalance  shown  in  record  C  is  similar 
to  that  observed  by  Cole  &  Curtis  with  their  external 
impedance  electrodes.  They  explained  their  data  as 
indicating  that  at  the  peak  of  activity  there  occurs  a 
200-fold  increase  in  the  membrane  conductance.  In 
the  squid  giant  axon,  the  membrane  conductance 
stays  above  the  resting  level  for  some  time  after  the 
end  of  the  falling  phase  of  the  action  potential. 

In  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber  of  the  frog,  the  im- 
pedance measurement  is  complicated  by  the  fact 
that  the  change  in  the  membrane  impedance  takes 
place  only  at  the  node  (133).  An  example  of  simul- 
taneous recording  of  the  action  current  and  of  the 
membrane  impedance  in  a  single  node  is  shown  in 
figure  1 3.  A  quantitati\'e  analysis  of  this  data  is  com- 
plicated by  the  fact  that  the  bridge  a.c.  flows  readily 
through  the  myelin  sheath  because  of  its  capacity. 
.Some  quantitative  information  in  regard  to  the  con- 
ductance at  the  peak  of  activity  can  be  obtained  by 
passing  testing  current  pulses  through  the  node  and 
comparing  the  change  in  the  membrane  potential  due 
to  the  current  pulse  before  and  during  activity.  It  has 
been  shown  by  this  method  that  at  the  peak  of  ac- 
tivity the  membrane  conductance  increases  approxi- 
mately 10  times.  In  the  nodal  membrane,  there  is  a 
close  parallelism  between  the  time  course  of  the  action 
potential  and  the  time  course  of  the  loss  in  the  mem- 
brane impedance  (129,  133);  in  this  respect  the  nodal 
membrane  is  in  sharp  contrast  with  the  squid  axon 
membrane. 

More  recently,  Hodgkin,  Huxley  &  Katz  (57,  58, 


FIG.  13.  Simultaneous  recording  of  action  potentials  and 
changes  in  the  membrane  impedance  during  activity  of  a 
single  node  of  Ranvier.  In  the  left-hand  record,  the  bridge  was 
balanced  for  the  impedance  at  rest;  in  the  right-hand  record, 
the  best  balance  was  obtained  near  the  peak  of  activity.  [From 
Tasaki  &  Freygang  (129).] 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE  9 1 

OUT, ,  IN 

o         I  o— -oJT. 


0      -!-^==^^^"»-r 


A2 


XT 


^xouyz^v^^v^  V 


i L 


I 1 

o  o— 


Ai 


4: 


4 

|-  I 

2 

mA/cm* 

/ 

50 

100   ^ 

°        150 

_        ,.  n          1                1 

'   f-<  1 

1 

-2 
-4 

\ 

\ 
0 

/ 

V 

FIG.  14.  L'/i/)fr.- Arrangement  used  for  clamping  the  membrane 
potential  of  a  squid  giant  axon  along  rectangular  time  courses. 
This  circuit  is  slightly  different  from  that  used  by  Hodgkin 
et  al.  (61),  but  the  principle  is  the  same.  Ai  is  a  low-gain  differ- 
ential amplifier;  An,  a  high-gain  differential  amplifier  (1000 
times).  The  thick  portions  of  the  lines  in  the  axon  represent 
the  exposed  surface  of  the  metal  wire  electrodes.  The  distance 
between  the  two  partitions  (P)  was  8  mm.  (The  diameter  of 
the  axon  and  the  wire  drawn  in  the  diagram  is  dispropor- 
tionately large.)  Resistance  r  was  2.5  (sometimes  50  or  250) 
ohms.  Lower:  Relation  between  the  membrane  depolarization 
(F)  and  the  membrane  current  at  the  peak  of  the  inward 
surge  (/).  Near  V  =  o,  the  V-I  relationship  is  roughly  linear, 
but  its  slope  is  about  '250  °f  ''^'"  °f  'he  straight  line  on  the 
right-hand  side.  Temperature,  2  2°C.  The  labile  portion  of 
the  V-I  relation  shown  by  the  broken  line  represents  either  all- 
or-none  (probably  nonsynchronous)  responses  in  some  parts 
of  the  membrane  (the  patch  theory),  or  a  partial  increase  in 
the  conductance  uniformly  all  o\'er  the  membrane  (the  sodium 
theory). 


61)  measured  in  a  series  of  beautiful  experiments  the 
conductance  of  the  squid  axon  membrane  by  a  very 
direct,  theoretically  simple  method,  often  referred  to 
as  the  '  method  of  voltage  clamp'.  The  diagram  in  the 
upper  part  of  figure  14  illustrates  the  principle  of  the 
method. 

A  giant  axon  is  placed  across  three  pools  of  sea 
water  separated  by  two  narrow  partitions.  A  pair  of 
metal  wire  electrodes  is  thrust  through  the  axon;  one 
is  used  for  measuring  the  membrane  potential  (F) 
and  the  other  for  passing  currents  through  the  axon 


9^ 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


membrane.  The  uvo  lateral  pools  are  directly 
grounded  with  large  silver  wire  electrodes.  The  middle 
pool  is  also  grounded  but  through  a  resistor  (r)  of  a 
few  ohms.  When  a  current  is  sent  through  the  l^ng 
internal  electrode,  this  resistor  (/■}  is  traversed  by  a 
current  (/)  passing  through  the  axon  membrane  in 
the  middle  pool;  the  small  potential  drop  (/r)  is 
amplified  and  is  taken  as  the  measure  of  the  membrane 
current.  The  membrane  potential  is  measured  across 
the  axon  membrane  in  the  middle  pool.  The  circuits 
connected  to  the  axon  are  constructed  in  such  a 
manner  that  the  membrane  potential  (f)  can  be 
maintained  at  any  desired  level  by  an  automatic 
adjustment  of  the  membrane  current  (/). 

The  principle  of  the  automatic  control  of  the  mem- 
brane current  by  the  feed-back  mechanism  is  as 
follows.  In  the  diagram  of  figure  14,  Ai  is  a  preampli- 
fier which  transmits  the  membrane  potential  (I)  at 
its  input  to  one  of  the  inputs  of  a  differential  amplifier 
A-).  The  other  input  of  A.,  marked  i  in  the  figure,  is 
connected  to  a  source  of  rectangular  (or  other)  voltage 
pulses.  The  output  of  amplifier  A2  has  the  .same 
phase  as  that  of  input  i  and  opposite  to  that  of 
input  2. 

First  let  us  consider  the  case  in  which  input  i  is 
grounded.  When  membrane  potential  (T)  tends  to 


rise  by  some  intrinsic  process  in  the  axon,  the  poten- 
tial of  input  2  starts  to  rise  immediately.  This  po- 
tential is  then  amplified  and,  after  reversing  its 
polarity,  transmitted  to  the  long  wire  electrode  in  the 
axon.  This  immediately  causes  a  flow  of  an  inward 
membrane  current  which  lowers  the  membrane  po- 
tential (r).  As  a  consequence,  if  the  gain  of  Ao  is 
sufficiently  high,  any  change  in  the  membrane  po- 
tential (r)  can  be  almost  completely  suppressed  by  an 
automatic  control  of  the  membrane  current  (/).  In 
practice,  the  over-all  gain  of  this  feed-back  amplifier 
was  1000  to  3000. 

Next,  we  consider  the  case  in  which  the  potential 
of  input  I  of  amplifier  A2  varies  along  a  rectangular 
time  course.  .Xt  the  moment  when  the  potential  of 
input  I  starts  to  rise,  thert  is  a  sudden  flow  of  an  out- 
ward current  through  the  axon  membrane.  This  flow 
immediately  raises  the  membrane  potential  (!').  The 
rise  in  Fis  transmitted  to  input  2,  tending  to  lower  the 
output  voltage  of  A-i.  In  the  steady  state  there  is  a  flow 
of  a  constant  membrane  current  which  is  sufficient  to 
maintain  the  membrane  potential  at  the  constant 
level.  If  the  gain  of  Aj  is  unity,  the  time  course  of  the 
membrane  potential  (T)  reproduces  the  potential 
applied  to  input  i  fairly  accurately. 

The  records  furnished  in  figure  15  show  the  rela- 


FiG.  15.  Relationship  between  the  membrane  potential  (dotted  trace)  and  the  membrane  current 
(continuous  trace)  obser\ed  with  the  arrangement  of  fig.  14.  In  records  A  to  D,  the  membrane  po- 
tential was  'clamped'  along  rectangular  time  courses  by  automatic  adjustment  of  the  membrane 
current.  In  E  and  F,  rectangular  current  pulses  were  applied  through  the  current  electrode  and  the 
variation  in  the  membrane  potential  was  recorded  with  the  other  internal  electrode;  the  defection 
sensitivity  of  the  current  trace  is  20  times  as  high  as  in  other  records.  Blanking  of  the  potential  trace 
indicates  0.25  msec.  Temperature,  22 °C. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


93 


tionslup  between  the  membrane  putential  and  the 
membrane  current  as  revealed  Ijy  the  method  of 
voltage  clamp.  When  the  membrane  potential  is 
raised  suddenly  from  its  resting  le\el  to  a  new  level 
slightly  above  the  ordinary  threshold  (i.e.  abo\e  12 
to  15  mv)  and  is  maintained  at  this  constant  le\el 
(record  ^4),  it  is  found  that  the  membrane  is  trax'crsed 
by  a  current  which  flows  first  outward,  then  inward 
and  finally  outward  again.  The  first  phase  of  the  out- 
ward current  is  .so  short  that  it  is  .seen  as  a  mere  break 
in  (he  upper  (current)  trace  in  the  record.  The  second 
phase  of  an  inward  current  is  seen  as  a  downward 
deflection  in  the  record.  The  third  phase  of  a  steady 
flow  of  an  outward  current  is  shown  by  the  current 
trace  staying  above  the  zero  level  in  the  right-hand 
side  of  the  record. 

The  obvious  explanation  of  the  time  course  of  the 
membrane  current  in  records  A  and  B  is  as  follows. 
The  a.xon  membrane  has  a  capacity  of  the  order  of 
I  ^f  per  cm-  (p.  85).  In  order  to  shift  the  membrane 
potential  suddenly  by  an  amount  (',  a  total  charge  of 
C-  r  (where  C  is  the  capacity  of  the  memijrane  in  the 
middle  pool)  has  to  be  supplied  by  the  current 
electrode.  This  capacitative  flow  of  current  takes 
place  within  the  extremely  short  period  of  time  during 
which  the  membrane  potential  is  actually  rising.  The 
second  phase  is  related  to  the  ability  of  the  membrane 
to  produce  an  action  potential  in  response  to  a  sudden 
rise  in  the  axoplasm  potential.  If  the  membrane 
potential  had  not  been  clamped  (as  in  fig.  15/^),  the 
potential  inside  the  axon  should  start  a  rapid  rise;  an 
inward  membrane  current  is  needed  to  counteract  this 
potential  ri.se  during  activity  and  to  maintain  the 
membrane  potential  at  the  constant  level.  The  third 
phase  of  the  membrane  current  reflects  the  situation 
in  which  a  relatively  strong  continuous  current  is 
needed  to  maintain  the  membrane  at  a  steady 
'depolarized'  level. 

When  the  voltage  step  in  the  clamping  rectangular 
pulse  is  increased,  the  intensity  of  the  inward  mein- 
brane  current  is  found  to  decrease.  The  relation 
between  the  depolarizing  voltage  step  and  the  peak  of 
the  inward  surge  of  current  is  plotted  in  the  lower 
part  of  figure  14.  When  the  voltage  step  is  approxi- 
mately equal  to  the  peak  value  of  the  meinbrane 
action  potential,  the  peak  of  the  inward  surge  is 
found  to  reach  zero  (fig.  15C).  As  the  voltage  step  is 
increased  further,  the  peak  stays  above  the  zero 
level;  i.e.  even  at  the  peak  of  the  inward  surge  of 
current,  the  membrane  current  is  in  the  direction 
imposed  by  the  applied  voltage.  As  can  be  seen  in 
the  figure,  the  relation  between  the  voltage  step   V 


and  the  current  /  at  the  peak  of  the  inward  surge  is 
represented  by  a  straight  line  in  a  wide  range  of 
voltage. 

The  fact  thai  the  \oltage-current  relation  is  linear 
can  he  taken  as  indicating  that,  in  thi^  range  of 
membrane  depolarization,  the  axon  membrane  be- 
haves like  a  '  battery'  with  a  definite  electromotive 
force  (emf)  and  a  definite  internal  resistance.  The 
voltage  at  which  there  is  no  current  flow  represents 
the  emf  of  this  i)attery  and  the  slope  of  the  voltage- 
current  straight  line  corresponds  to  the  internal 
resistance.  The  membrane  emf  at  the  peak  of  the 
inward  surge  of  current  coincides  with  the  peak  of 
the  membrane  action  potential.  In  the  experiments 
of  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (57,  p.  465),  the  membrane 
resistance  determined  from  the  slope  of  the  ]  -I  rela- 
tion is  about  30  ohm -cm'-.  The  figure  obtained 
recently  by  several  investigators  from  the  National 
Institutes  of  Health  is  7  to  12  ohm -cm- (at  i5to22°C). 
The  resistance  of  the  resting  membrane,  measured 
with  small  voltage  steps  (less  than  5  mv  or  negative 
voltages)  is  2  to  3  kl2-cm-  (61,  p.  440).  At  the  peak  of 
activity,  therefore,  the  membrane  conductance  is 
increased  by  a  factor  of  one  to  three  hundred.^ 

In  agreement  with  the  notion  that  the  inward  surge 
of  current  is  associated  with  the  ability  of  the  inem- 
brane  to  develop  an  action  potential,  narcosis  of  the 
axon  with  ethanol  or  urethane  is  known  to  eliminate 
the  inward  surge  reversibly.  A  recently  popularized 
method  of  reversible  elimination  of  the  action  po- 
tential is  to  reduce  the  sodium  concentration  of  the 
.surrounding  sea  water. 

The  finding  that  sodium  ions  are  necessary  in  the 
process  of  excitation  is  not  new.  More  than  half  a 
century  ago,  Overton  (97)  pointed  out  that  the  frog 
nerve-mu.scle  preparation  loses  its  ability  to  respond 
to  stimuli  unless  there  are  .sodium  or  lithium  ions  in 
the  medium.  He  also  pointed  out  that  chloride  ions  in 
Ringer  can  be  replaced  with  bromide,  nitrate,  ace- 
tate, salicylate,  etc.  without  eliminating  the  excita- 
bilit\-.  Recently  Hodgkin  &  Katz  (62)  have  shown  the 
importance  of  sodium  ions  in  a  inore  quantitative 
manner  [cf  also  Huxle>-  &  Stampfli  (68)  ].  They  have 
found  that  the  spike  amplitude  of  the  squid  giant  axon 

*  Quite  recently  similar  voltage-clamp  experiments  were 
carried  out  on  single  node  preparations  of  the  toad.  It  was 
observed  that  the  voltage-current  relationship  obtained  was 
similar  to  that  shown  in  figure  1 4  except  that  the  labile  portion 
of  the  cur%'e  indicated  by  the  broken  line  was  limited  in  a 
narrower  voltage  range.  The  membrane  conductance  deter- 
mined by  this  method  was  approximately  10  times  as  high  as 
that  of  the  resting  nodal  membrane. 


94 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


decreases  with  the  logarithm  of  the  external  sodium 
concentration,  the  proportionality  constant  being  very 
close  to  58  mv  which  is  the  coefficient  of  Nernst's 
equation  (cf.  p.  1 1 7). 

Based  on  this  and  other  experimental  facts,  Hodgkin 
&  Huxley  (59)  formulated  a  hypothesis  in  which  the 
inward  surge  is  interpreted  as  the  consequence  of  an 
increase  in  the  membrane  permeability  specific  to 
sodium  ions.  We  shall  discuss  this  point  later  (p.  1 18). 


THRESHOLD    AND    SUBTHRESHOLD    PHENOMENA 

In  the  early  part  of  this  century  when  physiolo- 
gists had  no  way  of  directly  observing  the  potential 
difference  across  the  excitable  membrane,  a  great 
number  of  articles  were  published  dealing  with  the 
problem  of  threshold  excitation  of  the  nerve  or  the 
muscle.  At  first,  physiologists  were  charmed  by  the 
elegant  physicomathematical  scheme  of  the  ionic 
theory  of  nerve  excitation  formulated  by  Nernst  (91). 
He  derived  the  relation  between  the  threshold  in- 
tensity of  current  and  its  duration  on  the  assuinption 
that  excitation  took  place  when  the  concentration  of 
some  ion  reached  a  certain  critical  level  near  the 
semipermeable  membrane  of  the  nerve.  Nernst 
argued  that  the  passage  of  an  electric  current  through 
a  uniform  electrolytic  conductor  in  the  nerve  cannot 
bring  about  any  electrochemical  changes  (except  for 
raising  temperature)  that  might  be  responsible  for 
initiation  of  an  impulse.  His  argument  is  ba.sed  upon 
the  principles  of  electrolytic  conductors  and  un- 
doubtedly it  is  still  valid  at  present.  Nevertheless, 
physiologists  .soon  abandoned  Nernst's  approach  to 
the  problem  and  accepted  more  formal,  physico- 
chemically  vague  arguments  which  reached  a  climax 
with  Monnier-Rashevsky-Hill  theory  ol  nerve  excita- 
tion (48,  88,  102). 

At  present  it  is  possible  to  pass  rectangular  pulses  of 
current  uniformly  through  the  excitable  membrane  of 
the  nerve  fiber,  and  to  determine  how  the  membrane 
potential  behaves  when  the  stimulus  reaches  thresh- 
old. The  assumptions  adopted  by  previous  investi- 
gators can  thus  be  subjected  to  direct  tests. 

Threshold  Membrane  Potential 

The  excitable  membrane  at  the  node  of  Ranvier  of 
the  vertebrate  myelinated  nerve  fiber  is  a  narrow 
ring-shaped  band.  Its  width  (0.5  to  i  m)  is  far  smaller 
than  the  diameter  of  the  fiber  at  the  node  or  than  the 
distance  between  neighboring  nodes.  It  is  possible  to 


record  potential  changes  across  this  membrane  by  the 
use  of  a  positive  feed-back  amplifier  [e.g.  McNichoI 
&  Wagner  (87)]. 

At  the  top  of  figure  16  is  shown  the  experimental 
arrangement  used  to  siud\  the  behavior  of  the  nodal 
meiTibrane  in  threshold  excitation.  The  fiber  is 
mounted  across  three  pools  of  saline  solution  separated 
by  two  air  gaps.  The  large  pool,  where  node  Nn  in  the 
figure  is  immersed,  is  filled  with  a  dilute  cocaine- 
Ringer's  solution.  The  pool  in  the  middle,  where  the 
node  under  study,  N,,  is  located,  is  filled  with  normal 
Ringer's  solution.  In  the  small  pool,  filled  with 
cocaine-Ringer's  or  an  isosmotic  pota.ssium  chloride 
solution,  the  small  portion  of  the  nerve  fiber  including 
N;  is  immersed.  The  electrode  in  the  large  pool  is 
connected  to  a  source  of  a  .square  voltage  pulse.  The 
middle  pool  is  grounded,  and  the  smallest  pool  is 
connected  to  the  high  impedance  input  of  a  positive 
feed-back  amplifier.  .Since  there  is  practicalK  no 
current  in  the  portion  of  the  fiber  in  the  air  gap 
between  Ni  and  No,  the  potential  measured  by  the 
amplifier  approximates  the  potential  drop  across  the 
nodal  membrane  of  Ni.  A  rectangular  voltage  pulse 


FIG.  16.  Demonstration  of  the  constancy  of  the  threshold 
membrane  potential  in  stimulation  of  a  single  node  of  Ranvier 
(Ni)  with  rectangular  \oltage  pulses  (S).  Nodes  N,,  and  N2 
are  inexcitablc.  V  indicates  the  input  of  a  positive  feed-back 
amplifier  for  recording  the  membrane  potential.  In  each  record 
the  stimulus  intensity  and  duration  used  are  given.  [From 
Tasaki  (1^6).] 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


95 


applied  between  No  and  Ni  sets  up  through  the  mem- 
brane of  Ni  a  current,  the  time  course  of  which  is 
distorted  by  current  flow  through  the  myelin  sheath. 

The  records  in  figure  i6  show  the  behavior  of  the 
membrane  potential  at  threshold  as  observed  with 
this  arrangement.  The  duration  of  the  stimulating 
pulse  was  varied  in  the  range  between  0.05  and  6.4 
msec.  At  every  stimulus  duration  the  stimulus  inten- 
sity was  adjusted  to  threshold,  and  without  changing 
the  intensity,  five  to  seven  sweeps  of  the  oscillograph 
beam  were  superposed  on  each  record.  Because  of 
spontaneous  variation  in  the  property  of  the  nerve 
fiber  (14,  99),  the  node  sometimes  responded  with  a 
full-sized  action  potential  and  sometimes  failed  to 
produce  an  action  potential. 

We  may  define  the  'threshold  membrane  potential' 
as  the  highest  potential  level  of  the  membrane  which, 
after  the  end  of  the  applied  stimulating  pulse,  decays 
without  producing  an  action  potential  (63,  126).  The 
level  of  the  threshold  membrane  potential  measured 
from  the  resting  potential  level  is  often  called  the 
'threshold  (or  critical)  depolarization.'  It  is  seen  in 
the  records  that  the  threshold  depolarization  is 
practically  independent  of  the  stimulus  duration. 
When  the  duration  is  short  (e.g.  0.05  msec),  a  very 
large  voltage  (200  mv)  is  needed  to  excite  the  node; 
the  observed  fact  is  that  this  high  a  voltage  is  required 
to  raise  the  membrane  potential  within  a  short  period 
of  time  to  the  threshold  level,  which  is  about  15  mv 
above  the  resting  potential.  This  is  exactly  what  has 
been  assumed  in  most  of  the  classical  theories  of  nerve 
excitation. 

As  we  have  discus.sed  in  a  previous  section,  the 
surface  membranes  of  the  nerve  fiber,  both  the  myelin 
sheath  and  the  nodal  membrane,  have  relatively  large 
capacities.  Consequently,  in  order  to  raise  the  mem- 
brane potential  by  a  constant  amount,  higher  stimu- 
lus intensities  are  required  at  shorter  stimulus  dura- 
tions. 

However,  there  is  in  this  type  of  experiment  one 
complication  that  has  not  been  fully  understood  by 
previous  investigators  who  worked  only  on  nerve 
trunks.  It  is  the  gradual  rise  in  the  membrane  po- 
tential that  precedes  the  rapid  rising  phase  of  the 
action  potential  in  stimulation  by  a  long  pulse  (see 
fig.  16,  record  for  6.4  msec).  In  response  to  a  long 
stimulating  pulse,  an  action  potential  either  appears 
within  a  few  msec,  (within  10  msec,  at  the  most) 
after  the  start  of  the  pulse  or  fails  to  appear  at  all. 
When  the  action  potential  fails  to  appear,  the  be- 
havior of  the  membrane  potential  does  not  diverge 
from  what  is  expected  from  the  physical  constants  of 


the  resting  nerve  fiber.  When  the  membrane  potential 
starts  to  diverge  distinctly  from  the  simple  time 
course,  provided  that  the  applied  pulse  has  not  been 
withdrawn  within  5  msec,  or  so,  there  is  alw-ays  (at 
least  in  a  normal  node)  an  action  potential. 

Action  potentials  evoked  by  long  stimulating 
pulses  have  a  more-or-less  gradual  rising  phase 
followed  by  a  phase  of  rapid  potential  rise.  If  the 
applied  stimulating  pulse  is  withdrawn  before  the 
start  of  the  rapid  potential  rise,  the  production  of 
a  full-sized  action  potential  is  prevented.  Such  a 
gradual  potential  rise  followed  liy  a  sudden  potential 
fall  caused  by  a  withdrawal  of  the  applied  pulse  is 
seen  in  the  record  labelled  46  mv  (1.6  msec.)  in 
figure  16. 

The  nonlinear  phenomenon  just  described  is  con- 
sidered at  present  to  indicate  the  following.  The  pro- 
duction of  an  action  potential  is  a  kind  of 'regenerative' 
or  'autocatalytic'  process  similar  to  the  explosion 
induced  by  heating  of  a  mass  of  gunpowder  (105). 
The  heat  applied  from  outside  causes  combustion  in 
only  some  of  the  gunpowder  particles;  the  heal  arising 
from  these  particles  in  turn  induces  combustion  in 
other  neighboring  particles.  Similarly,  when  the 
stimulus  duration  is  sufficiently  long,  the  start  of  a 
■  response'  (the  start  of  comljustion  in  the  analogy 
above)  tends  to  raise  the  membrane  potential  (tem- 
perature) together  with  the  applied  stimulus  (applied 
heat).  If  the  external  source  of  current  (heat)  is 
maintained,  this  process  eventually  raises  the  mem- 
brane potential  (temperature)  to  a  critical  explosive 
point.  If,  however,  the  applied  pulse  is  withdrawn 
before  the  critical  level  of  the  membrane  potential  is 
reached,  the  potential  returns  to  its  resting  level 
along  a  variable  time  course.  With  very  short  current 
pulses,  the  membrane  potential  has  to  be  raised  by 
the  external  source  up  to  the  critical  level.  ^ 

In  the  excitation  of  the  invertebrate  axon  with 
rectangular  current  pulses,  results  similar  to  those  in 
figui-e  16  have  been  obtained  by  several  investigators 
[e.g.  Hodgkin  &  Rushton  (63)].  To  stress  the  similar- 
ity between  the  vertebrate  myelinated  ner\e  fiber 
and  the  squid  a.xon,  unpublished  records  obtained 
by  Hagiwara  and  others  are  presented  in  figure  1 7. 
The  arrangement  of  the  stimulating  and  recording 
electrodes  used  is  similar  to  that  in  figure  14;  two 
metal  wires  about  30  mm  in  length  were  inserted 
along  the  axis  of  an  axon.  Pulses  of  constant  current 

'  It  should  be  pointed  out  that  some  physiologists  have 
slightly  different  viewpoints  in  regard  to  the  statement  made 
in  this  sentence  (104,  107). 


96 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV  -^  NEUROPHYSKJLOGV    I 


RECTANGULAR     CURRENT   PULSES 


SLOWLY   INCREASING  CURRENT  PULSES 


5  msec 


FIG.  17.  Upper  portion:  Stimulation  of  a  squid  giant  axon  by  rectangular  current  pulses  applied 
through  a  long  intracellular  metal  electrode.  The  membrane  potential  was  recorded  with  another 
intracellular  electrode.  Stimulus  durations  used  are  indicated  by  the  bars  in  the  records.  Lower  por- 
tion: Stimulation  of  a  squid  giant  axon  by  slowly  rising  current  pulses.  The  time  courses  of  the  current 
pulses  used  are  indicated  by  the  broken  lines.  [From  S.  Hagiwara  et  al.,  unpublished.] 


were  applied  through  one  of  the  internal  wire  elec- 
trodes and  the  change  in  the  membrane  potential 
was  recorded  with  the  other  electrode.  Under  these 
experimental  conditions,  the  axon  memiarane  is 
traversed  by  the  applied  current  uniformly  over  the 
whole  area  under  investigation.  The  intensity  of  the 
stimulating  pulses  was  adjusted  to  the  threshold  at 
every  stimulus  duration. 

It  is  seen  in  the  figure  that  the  threshold  membrane 
potential  defined  as  the  highest  subthreshold  level  of 
the  membrane  potential  is  approximately  constant 
(within  about  5  per  cent),  irrespective  of  the  stimulus 
duration.  As  in  the  nodal  membrane  of  the  toad 
myelinated  nerve  fiber,  the  decay  of  the  membrane 
potential  in  barely  subthreshold  stimulation  is  ex- 
tremely variable.  In  response  to  long  current  pulses 
(see  record  £)),  however,  a  phenomenon  we  have  not 
discussed  before  is  seen.  A  barely  subthreshold,  long 
current  pul.se  sets  up  an  approximately  exponential 
change  at  the  beginning;  later,  in  spite  of  maintained 
flow  of  the  constant  current,  the  memijrane  potential 
is  found  to  fall  gradually.  This  is  the  behavior  of  the 
membrane  associated  with  the  phenomenon  classi- 
cally known  as  '  accomodation'  [see  Erlanger  &  Blair 
(27)].  In  the  nodal  membrane,  the  process  of  accom- 
modation progresses  more  slowly  than  in  the  squid 
axon  and  is  not  apparent  in  figure  16. 


It  has  been  known  for  many  decades  (79)  that  a 
slowly  increasing  current  fails  to  excite  a  nerve  fiber 
even  when  its  intensity  rises  well  abo\e  the  rheobase.^ 
Evidently,  this  phenomenon  is  related  to  the '  accom- 
modative fall  in  the  membrane  potential'  just  men- 
tioned. This  point  is  illustrated  by  the  records  in 
the  lower  part  of  figure  i  7.  When  the  rate  of  current 
increase  is  greater  than  a  certain  critical  \alue,  a 
full-sized  action  potential  starts  when  the  membrane 
potential  reaches  the  threshold  level.  When  the 
membrane  current  rises  .slower  than  the  critical 
rate,  the  potential  begins  to  fall  while  the  current 
intensity  is  increasing.  Once  such  an  accommodative 
fall  in  the  membrane  potential  has  taken  place,  the 
potential  can  rise  well  above  the  ordinary  threshold 
le\el  without  initiating  an  action  potential. 

Now,  let  us  turn  to  the  corresponding  obsersation 
on  the  toad  myelinated  nerve  fiber.  Figure  18  shows 
the  beha\ior  of  the  nodal  membrane  in  threshold 
stimulation  bv  linearlv  rising  \oltage  pulses.  The 
experimental  arrangement  used  is  the  same  as  that 
used  in  the  experiment  of  figure  16.  Since  there  is  a 
high  ohmic  resistance  in  the  axis-cylinder  between 

"  This  is  the  threshold  for  a  long  rectangular  pulse.  For 
pulses  longer  than  5  msec,  the  threshold  is  practically  in- 
dependent of  duration. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


97 


Fio.  1 8.  Variations  of  the  membrane  potential  of  a  single 
node  (V)  caused  by  linearly  rising  voltage  pulses  (S).  The 
arrangement  of  fig.  i6  was  used.  In  records  A'  and  B'  the  miss- 
ing portions  of  the  potential  trace  (V)  indicate  production  of 
action  potentials  of  about  lOO  mv  in  amplitude.  The  trace  for 
the  stimulating  voltage  (S)  was  blanked  at  too  cps.  Large 
motor  nerve  fiber  of  the  toad.  Temperatine,  1 1  °C.  [From 
Tjisaki  (127).] 

nodes  No  and  Ni  and  since  the  time  constant  of  the 
membrane  is  far  shorter  than  the  time  scale  employed 
in  these  observations,  the  time  course  of  the  current 
through  the  nodal  membrane  is  similar  to  that  of  the 
applied  \0lta5e.  In  records  .-1,  B,  Cand  D,  an  accoino- 
dative  fall  in  the  membrane  potential  is  evident. 
Each  of  the  paired  records.  A- A'  or  B-B',  was  taken 
at  almost  the  same  stimulus  intensity;  in  one  QA  or  5) 
the  node  failed  to  respond,  and  in  the  other  (J'  or 
B')  a  large  action  potential  was  evoked.  The  peak 
value  of  the  subthreshold  membrane  potential  in 
these  cases  is  more  erratic  than  in  the  experiment  of 
figure  16;  it  is  roughly  independent  of  the  rise  time  of 
these  stimuli. 

In  most  classical  theories  of  nerve  excitation  [e.g. 
Hill  C48)],  the  process  of  accommodation  has  been 
regarded  as  a  gradual  rise  in  the  threshold  level  of 
the  nerve  during  the  period  of  prolonged  d.c.  stimula- 
tion. The  direct  observations  mentioned  above 
indicate  that  this  is  not  exactly  the  case.  It  is  due  to  a 
secondary  change  in  the  property  of  the  membrane 
which  decreases  the  effectiveness  of  the  current  to 
raise  the  membrane  potential.  Undoubtedly,  this  is 
related  to  the  phenomenon  of  delayed  rectification 
described  first  by  Cole  (18);  he  found  that  the  axon 
membrane  of  the  squid  shows  a  resistance  to  an 
outward  directed  maintained  current  far  smaller  than 


that  measured  with  an  inward  current  [see  also 
Hodgkin  (53)].  Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (59)  attributed 
this  process  mainly  to  an  increased  permeability  of 
the  membrane  to  potassium  ions.  In  the  nodal  mem- 
brane, there  is  some  e\idence  indicating  that  there  is 
a  change  in  the  resting  potential  when  the  membrane 
undergoes  an  accommodative  change  (127). 

Strength-Duialion  Rilalum 

The  relation  between  the  threshold  intensity  of  a 
stimulus  and  its  duration  is  called  a  strength-duration 
or  intensity-time  relation.  In  the  squid  giant  axon 
excited  by  means  of  a  long  internal  metal  wire  elec- 
trode, the  significance  of  this  relation  is  now  very 
clear.  When  a  rectangular  pulse  of  current  is  applied 
to  the  membrane  through  the  internal  electrode,  the 
inembrane  potential  rises  exponentially  as  described 
by  equation  (4-2).  If  a  stimulus  which  lasts  no  longer 
than  about  2  msec'  (at  i4°C)  is  to  initiate  an  action 
potential,  the  membrane  potential  has  to  reach  the 
critical  level,  l\,  at  the  end  of  the  pulse.  This  leads  to 
the  relation 

F,  =  IRii   -  e-J-'Rf), 

in  which  T  is  the  duration  of  the  current  pulse,  /  is 
the  current  intensity  and  RC  the  time  constant  of  the 
membrane.  Rearranging  the  terms,  we  have 

/ 

T  =  RC  log . 

This  is  known  as  Blair's  equation  for  strength- 
duration  relation  (15).  Because  of  the  interaction 
between  the  stimulating  current  and  the  response  of 
the  membrane  mentioned  abo\e,  this  equation  is 
only  a  poor  approximation  near  the  rheobase. 

Stimulation  of  a  squid  giant  a.xon  through  a  glass 
pipette  can  be  treated  in  a  similar  fashion  by  using  the 
solution  of  the  cable  equation  for  the  corresponding 
conditions.  Again  the  rheobase  will  be  slightly  (20 
to  30  per  cent)  smaller  than  that  expected  from  the 
space  and  time  constants  of  the  resting  axon  mem- 
brane. When  the  duration  becomes  far  shorter  than 
the  membrane  time  constant,  another  complication 
(related  to  the  phenomenon  of  abolition  of  an  action 
potential  to  be  discussed  in  the  next  section)  prob- 
ably sets  in.  When  the  current  pulse  is  extremely 
short,  the  uncharged  membrane  on  both  sides  of  the 
site  of  stimulation  is  expected  to  prevent  a  further  rise 
in  potential  at  the  site  of  stimulation  and  to  suppress 
the  start  of  an  action  potential.  These  factors  have 
not  yet  been  carefully  investigated. 

'  This  figure  was  kindly  supplied  by  Dr.  S.  Hagiwara. 


98 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


In  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber,  the  strength- 
duration  relation  is  determined  primarily  by  the 
complicated  network  formed  by  the  nodal  membrane, 
the  axis  cylinder  and  the  myelin  sheath.  Because  of 
the  interaction  between  the  applied  current  and  the 
start  of  a  response,  the  rheobase  is  20  to  30  per  cent 
smaller  than  that  expected  from  the  membrane 
properties  at  rest  (see  fig.  16).  So  far,  no  one  has 
derived  the  equation  describing  the  distribution  of 
the  membrane  potential  caused '  by  a  rectangular 
voltage  applied  at  one  point  between  two  neighboring 
nodes.  In  practice,  however,  the  strength-duration 
relation  is  expressed  by  a  purely  empirical  formula: 


.=  .(.+^). 


in  which  .S'  is  the  threshold  voltage,  /;  the  rheobase 
voltage  and  a  a  constant  which  has  a  dimension  of 
time  and  is  known  as'chronaxie'.  It  is  known  that  the 
chronaxie  for  a  node  varies  markedly  with  the  distance 
between  the  node  under  study  and  the  stimulating 
partition  (65). 

Subthreshold  Response 

It  has  been  shown  in  the  explanations  of  figures  16 
and  1 7  that  the  membrane  potential  raised  by  a 
brief  shock  of  barely  subthreshold  intensity  decays 
along  a  variable  time  course  which  is  far  slower  than 
that  expected  from  the  physical  properties  of  the 
resting  nerve  fiber.  This  delay  in  the  fall  of  the  mem- 
brane potential  is  said  to  be  due  to  a  "subthreshold 
response'  or  a  'local  response'.  Such  delay  occurs  only 
when  the  stimulus  intensity  is  greater  than  80  to  90 
per  cent  of  the  threshold  (in  single  node  preparations). 
This  phenomenon  is  more  marked  in  a  preparation 
with  high  threshold  and  a  poor  action  potential  than 
in  a  fresh  normal  preparation.  The  phase  of  the 
potential  rise  in  these  cases  is  determined  bv  the 
physical  properties  of  the  resting  membrane.  The 
subthreshold  response  is  considered  as  a  sign  of  the 
beginning  of  the  regenerative  process  which  has 
subsided  without  growing  into  a  full-sized  response. 
The  historical  aspect  of  the  concept  of  the  sub- 
threshold response  has  been  discussed  in  the  intro- 
duction of  this  chapter. 

A  subthreshold  'response'  is  different  from  an 
ordinary  full-sized  response  in  that  it  does  not  leave 
behind  it  a  clear  refractoriness.  In  the  period  during 
which  the  membrane  potential  stays  above  the  level 
of  the  resting  potential,  the  threshold  for  the  second 
shock  (necessary   to   evoke   a   full-sized   response)   is 


lower  than  the  threshold  at  rest.  (In  the  squid  axon,  a 
subthreshold  response  is  followed  by  a  small  '  under- 
shoot', during  which  the  membrane  potential  is  below 
the  resting  level;  the  threshold  is  higher  in  this  period 
than  at  rest.)  Like  an  ordinary  response,  a  sub- 
threshold response  is  associated  with  a  reduction  in 
the  membrane  impedance;  the  reduction  is,  however, 
far  smaller  than  that  associated  with  a  full-sized 
response  (see  fig.  12). 

The  arriplitude  of  the  full-sized  action  potential 
depends  slightly  on  whether  or  not  it  is  preceded  by  a 
marked  subthreshold  response.  It  is  seen  in  the  rec- 
ords of  figure  16  that  the  action  potentials  preceded 
by  a  slow  gradual  potential  rise  are  consistently 
smaller  than  those  preceded  by  an  abrupt  potential 
rise.  Because  of  this  variation  in  the  amplitude  of  the 
response  and  of  the  subthreshold  responses,  the 
response  recorded  at  the  site  of  stimulation  is  said  to 
be  only  approximately  all-or-none. 

In  the  experiments  of  figures  16  and  17,  the  stimu- 
lating current  is  applied  uniformly  through  the  ex- 
citable inembrane.  It  is  not  possible,  therefore,  to 
interpret  the  subthreshold  response  as  an  action  po- 
tential localized  in  a  small  area  subjected  to  a  strong 
stimulating  current  (see  p.  76).  This  area  hypothesis 
of  the  subthreshold  response  can  be  saved  if  one 
assumes  that  the  surface  of  the  excitable  membrane  is 
not  uniform  but  that  there  are  spots  or  patches  where 
the  sensitivity  to  electric  stimuli  is  higher  than  at  the 
remaining  surface.  In  the  sodium  theory  (59),  a  sub- 
threshold response  is  attributed  to  a  small  increase  in 
the  sodium  conductance  of  the  membrane. 

When  a  nerve  fiber  is  excited  by  a  stimulating  cur- 
rent distributed  nonuniformly  over  the  membrane,  the 
time  course  of  the  subthreshold  response  is  compli- 
cated by  the  spatial  factor.  Especially  when  the  state 
of  the  nerve  filler  has  been  altered  locally  by  the 
stimulating  or  recording  electrode  or  when  there  are 
large  stimulation  artifacts,  pictures  very  different 
from  those  in  figures  16  and  17  can  be  obtained.  Be- 
cause of  these  complications,  there  have  been  a  num- 
ber of  confusing  reports  on  this  topic. 

Measurement  oj  Excitability  by  L  "sing  Test  Shocks 

In  classical  physiology  writers  used  to  .speak  of 
measuring  the  'excitability'  of  the  nerve  by  test 
shocks.  Insofar  as  we  define  the  excitability  as  the  re- 
ciprocal of  the  threshold  (p.  80),  this  procedure  of 
measuring  the  excitability  is  simple  and  straight- 
forward. It  seems,  however,  that  to  old  phy.siologists 
the    term    'e.\cital3ilit\'    or    '  irritabilitv'    had    some 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


99 


anthropomorphized  meaning  [e.g.  Verworn  (140)] 
and  the  procedure  of  measuring  it  was  more-or-less 
comparable  to  determining  a  man's  ability  by 
mental  tests.  Such  a  concept  of  excitability  has  no 
clear  physiological  meaning. 

Here,  we  shall  discuss  the  significance  of  the  method 
of  using  test  shocks  to  explore  the  state  of  the  nerve 
fiber.  This  method  has  been  used  mainly  on  verte- 
brate nerve  fibers. 

In  the  arrangement  illustrated  in  the  inset  in  figure 
19,  an  isolated  nerve  fiber  is  mounted  across  two  pools 
of  Ringer's  solution.  The  narrow  air  gap  is  located 
between  nodes  Ni  and  No.  Through  the  electrodes  im- 
mersed on  the  pools,  short  pulses  superposed  on  long 
rectangular  \-oltage  pulses  are  applied  to  the  fiber. 
The  intensity  of  the  short  pulse,  .S',  the  voltage  of  the 
long  pulse,  V,  and  the  time  interval,  /,  from  the  begin- 
ning of  the  long  pulse  to  the  start  of  the  short  pulse 
are  three  variables  in  this  experiment.  The  data  pre- 
sented in  A  were  obtained  by  fixing  voltage,  v,  at  one 
of  four  different  values  (  —  20,  —10,  10  and  20  mv) 
and  adjusting  S  to  make  the  composite  stimulating 
pulses  barely  eflfective  in  eliciting  a  nerve  impulse  at 
varying  values  of  /.  The  data  in  B  were  obtained  by 
fixing  t  at  2  msec,  and  adjusting  v  and  .S*  to  make  the 
pulses  barely  effective.  Thresholds  were  determined 
by  taking  the  response  of  the  muscle  innervated  by 
the  nerve  fiber  as  an  index  of  initiation  of  an  im- 
pulse; the  same  result,  however,  can  \)e  obtained  by 


taking  the  action  potential  of  the  nerve  fiber  as  an 
index. 

In  B,  the  observed  point  for  .S'  =  o  is  at  j)  =  30  to 
31  mv,  indicating  that  the  rheobasic  voltage  of  the 
fiber  under  these  experimental  conditions  was  about 
30  mv.  The  threshold  for  the  brief  shock  depends  on 
the  duration  of  the  shock;  for  durations  shorter  than 
about  30  /isec,  the  threshold  rises  inversely  as  the  du- 
ration. The  shock  used  in  the  present  experiment  was 
within  this  range  and  its  threshold  was  taken  as  unity. 

The  curves  in  A  show  how  the  threshold  for  the  test 
shock,  S,  is  modified  by  the  subthreshold  pulse,  v.  At 
any  fixed  value  of  /,  the  change  in  S  is  roughly  pro- 
portional to  I',  except  when  v  is  greater  than  about 
50  per  cent  of  the  rheobase  (5).  One  thing  that  looks 
strange  in  this  figure  at  first  sight  is  the  change  in 
threshold  observed  at  /  =  o  and  for  negative  values  of 
/.  This  is  a  constant  finding  in  single  fiber  experiments 
and  has  also  been  observed  by  Erlanger  &  Blair  in 
their  experiments  with  nerve  trunks  (27).  If  the  test 
shocks  measure  the  state  of  the  nerve  fiber  at  the 
moment  when  the  shocks  are  delivered,  it  is  obviously 
absurd  that  the  threshold  starts  to  change  before  the 
beginning  of  the  subthreshold  pulse  used  to  modify 
the  state  of  the  fiber. 

There  are  two  factors  that  serve  to  explain  this 
strange  fact.  One  factor  is  the  time  required  for  the 
spread  of  membrane  potential  along  the  myelin 
sheath,  and  the  other  factor  is  the  production  of  a 


<-t^ 


S/Sc 


1.2 


O  y 
/I/ 

■^1.0 


*^. 


0.8 


0.& 


}^ 


Mr 


N,     N, 


ZA  =  -20mV 
-10 


\ 


10 


.A A- 


20 


1- 


-J t 

20  msec 


\ 

I 

0.8 
0.4 


B 


s/s„ 

-1.2    - 

\ 


.s 


I 


2  msec 


,}^ 


:n 


o    1» 


A   '-I 


ir 


-20 


20 


40  mV 


FIG.  19.  Changes  in  threshold  for  a  brief  shock  (S)  caused  by  application  of  a  subthreshold  rec- 
tangular voltage  pulse  (v).  The  sign  of  the  stimulating  \oltage  pulse  is  positi\  e  when  the  pulse  induces 
an  outward  current  through  node  N-j  in  the  diagram.  S(,  represents  the  threshold  for  the  brief  shock 
alone. 


lOO  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


subthreshold  response.  We  shall  first  discuss  the  lime 
factor. 

When  a  rectangular  pulse  of  voltage  is  applied 
across  the  air  gap  in  the  arrangement  of  figure  ig,  the 
membrane  potential  at  the  nearest  node  (Ni  and  Nj) 
rises  (or  falls)  along  a  sigmoid  curve.  This  sigmoid 
time  course  arises  from  the  situation  that  both  the 
myelin  sheath  and  the  nodal  membrane  have  a 
capacity  which  delays  spread  of  the  membrane  po- 
tential. The  problem  of  spread  of  potential  along  a 
uniform  cable  is  discu.ssed  in  some  detail  on  p.  86. 
The  situation  in  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber  is  compli- 
cated by  the  discontinuities  at  the  nodes,  and  un- 
fortunately no  rigorous  mathematical  solution  of  the 
problem  is  at  present  available.  It  is  certain,  however, 
that  both  V  (the  membrane  potential  at  the  node) 
and  dV/dl  are  zero  at  /  =  o,  and  1' rises  first  gradually, 
then  faster  and  finally  approaches  the  plateau. 

The  variation  in  the  membrane  potential  caused  by 
a  brief  voltage  pulse  is  given  by  the  derivative,  dl'/dt, 
times  a  constant,  because  a  brief  pulse  can  be  re- 
garded as  a  diff"erence  between  two  long  rectangular 
pulses  of  the  same  intensity  but  starting  in  succession 
at  a  small  time  interval.  From  this  it  follows  that  the 
maximum  of  the  membrane  potential  change  caused 
at  the  node  by  the  test  shock  is  reached  a  certain 
period  of  time,  t,,,  after  the  delivery  of  the  shock  [see 
curve  1-/4  on  p.  492  of  Lorente  de  No  (77)].  This  time 
(to)  depends  on  the  distance  from  the  stimulating  par- 
tition to  the  node  under  study  (122).  Now,  in  the 
range  of  voltage,  v,  where  the  relationship  between  S 
and  !>  is  expressed  by  straight  line  I  in  figure  igfi, 
action  potentials  are  elicited  when  the  algebraic  sum 
of  the  potentials  caused  by  v  and  S  reaches  the 
critical  level.  Therefore,  the  origin  of  time  has  to  be 
shifted  to  the  left  by  to  if  the  curves  are  to  represent 
the  change  in  the  state  of  the  fiber  caused  by  the  sub- 
threshold pulse,  V.  The  argument  along  this  line  was 
developed  first  by  Erlanger  &  Blair  (14,  38)  and 
later  by  Tasaki  (118,  122). 

Next,  we  discuss  the  second  factor  that  has  to  be 
taken  into  consideration  in  the  analysis  of  the  curves 
in  A  of  figure  19.  W'hcn  the  test  shock,  S,  precedes  the 
start  of  the  subthreshold  pulse,  v,  the  change  in  the 
threshold  of  S  is  small.  It  has  been  mentioned  that, 
when  a  brief  shock  is  close  to  its  threshold,  the  fall  in 
the  membrane  potential  at  the  node  is  far  slower 
than  that  expected  from  the  physical  constants  of  the 
resting  nerve  fiber.  If  a  weak  (positive)  rectangular 
pulse  (j/)  follows  such  a  barely  subthreshold  test  pulse, 
it  is  possible  that  the  membrane  potential  is  raised 
to  the  critical  level,  thus  initiating  a  full-sized  response. 


This  can  account  for  a  decrease  in  threshold  in  the 
region  where  /  is  negative  and  v  is  positive.  Katz  (72) 
developed  this  argument  to  explain  the  results  of  his 
experiments  in  which  the  effect  of  a  brief  shock  was 
tested  by  another  brief  shock.  His  argument  is  not 
entirely  correct  since  he  ignored  the  first  (time)  factor 
mentioned  above.  Erlanger  &  Blair  as  well  as 
Tasaki  neglected  the  second  factor  arising  from  the 
subthreshold  phenomenon;  their  argument,  there- 
fore, has  to  be  partly  modified. 

Finally,  we  shall  discuss  the  significance  of  the 
break  in  the  v-S  relation  in  the  experiment  of  figure 
195.  Some  physiologists  believe  that  this  break  is  a 
sign  of  the  development  of  subthreshold  response  to  a 
subrheobasic  rectangular  pulse  alone  [e.g.  Nieder- 
gerke  (92)].  As  we  see,  however,  in  the  lower  right 
part  of  figure  16,  this  is  not  exactly  the  case.  The 
continuous  transition  from  straight  line  I  to  II  is  evi- 
dently due  to  the  interplay  of  the  two  stimuli  related 
to  the  development  of '  the  slowly  rising  phase  of  the 
membrane  potential'  which  precedes  a  full-sized  ac- 
tion potential. 


."iBOLlTION    OF    THE    .ACTION    POTENTI.AL 

Initiation  of  an  action  potential  can  be  regarded 
as  a  transition  of  the  membrane  from  its  resting  state 
into  the  active  state  which  is  characterized  by  a  low 
membrane  resistance  and  a  high  potential  level.  The 
reverse  process,  i.e.  a  transition  from  the  active  state 
of  the  membrane  to  the  resting  state,  was  first  demon- 
strated in  the  cardiac  muscle  of  the  kid  (142),  then 
in  the  toad  nodal  membrane  (126)  and  finally  very 
recently  in  the  squid  axon  membrane.  The  action  po- 
tential of  the  cardiac  muscle  is  associated  with  a 
systolic  contraction.  The  fact  that  this  contraction  can 
be  abolished  by  a  strong  (anodal)  current  pulse  in  an 
all-or-none  manner  has  been  known  since  the  time  of 
Biedermann  (12,  pp.  257-264). 

The  regenerative  process  of  initiating  an  action  po- 
tential is  set  off  by  a  change  (rise)  in  the  membrane 
potential  up  to  a  certain  level.  In  an  analogous  man- 
ner, the  process  of  abolition  of  an  action  potential  is 
set  off  by  a  change  (fall)  in  membrane  potential 
down  to  a  critical  level.  This  is  shown  in  figure  20. 
These  records  were  obtained  from  a  single  node 
preparation  of  the  toad.  The  arrangement  of  the 
stimulating  and  recording  electrodes  used  is  similar 
to  that  for  the  experiment  of  figure  16.  The  first  pulse 
of  outward  membrane  current  raises  the  membrane 
potential  to  the  level  slightly  above  the  critical  po- 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


FIG.  20.  Abolition  of  the  action  potential  of  a  single  node  by  pulses  of  inward  membrane  current. 
The  lower  trace  in  each  record  indicates  the  time  course  of  the  voltage  applied  between  No  and  Ni 
in  the  diagram  of  fig.  16,  top.  The  amplitude  of  the  recorded  action  potential  was  approximately  100 
mv.  Time  marks  in  msec.  A  toad  nerve  fiber  at  io°C.  [From  Tasaki  (126).] 


tential  necessary  to  initiate  an  action  potential.  The 
second  pulse  of  inward  current  is  applied  during;  the 
falling  phase  of  the  action  potential  and  lowers  the 
membrane  potential  down  to  various  levels. 

When  the  change  in  the  membrane  potential 
caused  by  the  second  pulse  is  slight  (records  B,  B',  C), 
the  potential  rises  after  the  end  of  the  pulse  back  to 
the  level  which  might  have  been  reached  if  the  sec- 
ond current  pulse  had  not  been  applied.  When  the 
membrane  potential  is  lowered  by  the  second  pulse 
below  a  certain  critical  level  (records  C,  D),  the  po- 
tential does  not  rise  after  the  end  of  the  pulse  but  falls 
further  to  the  potential  level  of  the  resting  membrane. 
At  the  critical  intensity  of  the  second  pulse  (record 
D'),  the  membrane  potential  in  .some  instances  rises 
to  the  level  of  the  active  membrane  and  in  others  ''alls 
to  the  level  of  the  resting  potential.  A  further  increase 
in  the  intensity  of  the  second  pulse  lowers  the  mem- 
brane potential  below  the  resting  potential  (record  E, 
E');  however,  after  the  end  of  the  pulse,  the  mem- 
brane potential  rises  and  settles  usually  at  the  level  of 
the  resting  potential. 

Similar  records  of  abolition  of  action  potentials 
have  been  taken  from  a  squid  giant  axon  which  has 
been  treated  with  intracellularly  injected  tetraethyl- 
ammonium  chloride.  This  chemical  when  applied  ex- 


ternally is  known  to  prolong  the  duration  of  the  ac- 
tion potential  of  the  frog  nerve  and  muscle  fiber  (46, 
78).  Prolonged  action  potentials  of  the  squid  or  of 
the  toad  motor  nerve  fiber  show  a  remarkable  resem- 
blance to  the  action  potential  of  the  heart  muscle. 
When  the  action  potential  is  prolonged  as  it  is  in  these 
cases,  the  time  constant  of  the  membrane  is  far 
shorter  than  the  duration  of  the  action  potential  and 
the  demonstration  of  the  phenomenon  of  abolition  is 
thereby  made  easy. 

It  is  seen  that  the  critical  potential  le\el  for  aboli- 
tion gradually  rises  during  activity.  Toward  the  end 
of  the  prolonged  action  potential,  the  critical  level  for 
abolition  is  close  to  the  level  of  the  '  shoulder'  of  the 
action  potential  at  which  the  membrane  potential 
starts  to  fall  rapidly. 

It  is  an  interesting  fact  that  the  action  potential 
which  has  been  abolished  in  its  very  early  phase 
leaves  behind  it  no  refractory  period.  This  is  shown 
by  the  superposed  record  in  figure  21.  Record  A  in 
the  figure  is  an  ordinary  unabolished  action  potential 
of  a  single  node  of  the  toad  motor  fiber.  When  this 
action  potential  is  abolished  in  its  later  stages  by  a 
pulse  of  inward  current  through  the  node  (record  B), 
there  is  a  relati\ely  refractory  period  following  this 
prematurely   terminated   response;   a   strong  current 


I02 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


FIG.  21.  Recovery  of  the  amplitude  of  the  action  potential 
following  abolition  of  a  response  of  a  single  node.  The  ar- 
rangement shown  by  the  diagram  in  the  upper  part  of  fig.  i6 
was  used.  .4.  Action  potential  of  a  single  node  (,top~)  and  a 
truncated  60  cycle  wave  indicating  100  mv  level  in  applied 
stimulating  and  abolishing  pulses  (^bollom).  B,  C  and  D.  Super- 
posed recordings  showing  recovery  after  an  abolished  response. 
Temperature,  io°C.  [From  Tasaki  (126).] 


pulse  is  needed  to  initiate  a  second  action  potential 
and  the  amplitude  of  the  second  response  decreases 
continuously  with  decreasing  interval  between  the 
two  responses.  Record  D  shows  that,  following;  the 
action  potential  abolished  at  its  peak,  the  node 
exhibits  no  refractoriness  to  the  following  stimulating 
pulse.  In  record  C,  the  action  potential  has  been 
abolished  after  the  potential  has  fallen  slightly  from 
the  peak;  it  is  seen  that  the  amplitude  of  the  second 
response  is  slightly  subnormal  at  the  beginning  and 
recovers  gradually. 

These  observations  reveal  how  the  process  respon- 
sible for  the  refractoriness  progresses  during  the  falling 
phase  of  the  action  potential.  As  was  pointed  out  by 
Adrian  (2)  in  1921,  the  end  of  the  action  potential 
coincides  roughly  with  the  beginning  ot  the  relatively 
refractory  period  [cf.  Tasaki  {119,  124)].  When  the 
action  potential  is  abolished  in  the  middle  of  its  falling 
phase,  the  recovery  in  the  amplitude  of  the  second 
response  starts  in  the  middle  of  the  normal  recovery 
curve  (126).  It  has  been  suggested  therefore  that  the 
refractoriness  is  due  to  some  chemical  product  which 
accumulates  during  the  falling  phase  of  the  action 
potential.  In  the  sodium  theory  (see  p.  1 18)  a  different 
explanation  is  given  to  the  origin  of  the  refractoriness. 

The  rapid  falling  phase  following  the  shoulder  of  a 
normal  action  potential  appears  to  be  a  transition  of 


the  membrane  from  the  active  state  to  the  resting 
state  resulting  from  the  gradually  rising  critical  level 
for  abolition  reaching  the  level  of  the  continuously 
falling  potential  level  of  the  membrane. 


NERVOUS    CONDUCTION    .iiLONG    UNIFORM    .AXONS 

We  are  now  ready  to  discuss  nervous  conduction  as 
a  process  that  involves  production  of  action  poten- 
tials in  successive  portions  of  the  surface  membrane  of 
the  nerve  fiber  in  an  orderly  fashion.  In  the  squid 
giant  axon,  the  rise  in  the  membrane  potential**  at  the 
peak  of  the  action  potential  is  100  to  120  mv  and  the 
critical  depolarization  necessary  to  initiate  an  action 
potential  is  12  to  15  mv.  Furthermore,  the  resistance 
of  the  membrane  in  the  active  area  is  far  smaller 
than  that  of  the  membrane  at  rest  (see  p.  89). 
Therefore,  when  a  portion  of  an  axon  membrane  is 
thrown  into  action  by  a  pulse  of  stimulating  current, 
the  adjacent  portion  of  the  membrane  is  automatically 
brought  to  action  Isy  the  restimulating  effect  of  the 
local  circuit  between  the  active  and  resting  area  of  the 
axon.  By  a  repetition  of  this  process  of  stimulation 
by  the  local  circuit,  the  activity  spreads  indefinitely  on 
both  sides  of  the  site  of  initial  stimulation. 

The  local  circuit  cannot  be  closed  if  there  is  no 
conducting  fluid  medium  outside  the  nerve  filler. 
Therefore,  nervous  conduction  is  expected  to  stop  if 
the  saline  solution  outside  the  fiber  is  completely  re- 
moved. In  practice,  it  is  not  possible  to  remove  the 
fluid  outside  the  fiber  completely,  but  it  is  easy  to  re- 
duce it  by  immersing  a  cleaned  single  nerve  fiber  in 
mineral  oil.  Hodgkin  (52)  has  found  that,  when  an 
isolated  nerve  fiber  of  the  cralo  is  immersed  in  mineral 
oil,  the  velocity  of  the  nerve  impulse  is  markedly  re- 
duced. This  is  a  clear-cut  demonstration  of  the  im- 
portance of  the  local  circuit  in  the  process  of  propaga- 
tion of  a  nerve  impulse. 

In  figure  22  a  set  of  records  from  Hodgkin's  paper 
is  reproduced.  The  velocity  of  the  crab  nerve  fiber  in 
normal  sea  water  was  4  to  5  m  per  sec.  This  was  re- 
duced by  20  to  40  per  cent  when  the  fiber  was  trans- 
ferred into  a  bath  of  mineral  oil.  This  reduction  in  the 

*  The  membrane  potential  is  defined  as  the  energy  required 
to  transfer  a  unit  charge  across  the  membrane  from  the  ex- 
ternal medium  to  the  axoplasm.  If  the  potential  difference 
between  the  fluid  in  the  intracellular  micropipette  and  the 
axoplasm  (which  is  probably  small  but  indeterminable)  is 
ignored,  this  coincides  with  the  potential  of  an  intracellular 
electrode  referred  to  the  medium.  Since  the  membrane  potential 
at  rest  is  a  negative  quantity,  a  small  rise  in  the  membrane 
potential  represents  a  decrease  in  its  absolute  magnitude. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


103 


FIG.  22.  Demonstration  ol  the  dependence  of  the  conduction 
velocity  of  a  crab  nerve  fiber  upon  the  resistance  of  the  external 
medium.  A  and  C.  Action  potential  recorded  with  sea  water 
covering  95  per  cent  of  the  intermediate  conduction  distance. 
B  and  D.  Fiber  completely  immersed  in  oil.  Conduction 
distance,  1 3  mm.  Time  in  msec.  [From  Hodgkin  (52).] 


velocity  was  prompt  and  completely  reversiijle;  there 
seems  to  be  little  doubt,  therefore,  that  the  effect  is  due 
to  the  increased  electric  resistance  of  the  surrounding 
medium. 

The  velocity  of  a  nerve  impulse  is  determined  by  a 
mechanism  involving  the  interplay  of  many  factors.  In 
a  uniform  axon  immersed  in  a  large  volume  of  highly 
conducting  fluid  medium,  the  mechanism  determin- 
ing the  conduction  velocity  is  as  follows.  In  the  inactive 
area  of  the  axon  ahead  of  the  active  area,  the  mem- 
brane is  traversed  by  an  outward  current  (see  fig.  23) 
the  intensity  of  which  depends  on  the  velocity  of  the 
impulse.  This  current  is  supplied  by  the  active  area 
immediately  behind  the  active-inactive  boundary. 
The  membrane  current  in  the  active  area  is  inward 
(see  fig.  23),  and  this  inward  current  tends  to  delay 
the  rate  of  potential  rise  in  the  active  region.  If  the 
membrane  is  capable  of  developing  an  action  poten- 
tial rapidly  in  spite  of  the  existence  of  a  strong  inward 
current,  the  velocity  tends  to  be  high.  If  the  capacity 
and  the  conductance  of  the  resting  membrane  are 
large,  the  active  area  of  the  membrane  has  to  supply 
a  strong  current  to  bring  the  membrane  potential  of 
the  inactive  area  up  to  the  critical  level,  and  conse- 


quently the  velocity  tends  to  be  small.  A  large  longi- 
tudinal resistance  (small  fiber  diameter)  is  expected  to 
have  the  same  effect  upon  the  velocity  as  an  increased 
external  resistance. 

Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (59)  determined  the  relation 
between  the  membrane  potential  and  the  membrane 
conductance  on  the  squid  axon.  By  using  the  cable 
equation  and  a  set  of  empirical  formulae  relating  the 
membrane  potential  and  the  membrane  conductance, 
they  calculated  the  velocity  and  obtained  a  solution 
of  the  right  order  of  magnitude. 

We  have  discussed  in  a  previous  section  (see  p.  83) 
the  cable  properties  of  a  uniform  invertebrate  axon. 
In  a  uniform  axon  carrying  an  impulse  of  a  constant 
velocity,  there  are  certain  features  that  deserve  further 
discussion.  First  of  all,  it  should  be  pointed  out  that 
there  is  an  inseparable  relationship  between  the 
spatial  distribution  of  the  membrane  potential  and 
the  time  course  of  the  action  potential.  A  diagram 
representing  the  time  course  of  an  action  potential 
can  be  converted  into  a  diagram  showing  the  spatial 
distribution  simply  by  converting  the  time  scale  into 
the  distance  scale  by  using  the  conduction  velocity  as 
a  conversion  factor.  This  and  the  following  statements 
are  not  applicable  to  axons  with  any  macroscopic 
nonuniformity  along  their  length  in  regard  to  the 
size  and  shape  of  the  action  potential. 

Next  to  be  discussed  is  the  relationship  between  the 
spatial  distribution  of  the  action  potential  and  the  dis- 
tribution of  the  longitudinal  and  the  membrane  cur- 
rent of  the  axon.  According  to  Ohm's  law,  the  longi- 
tudinal current  in  the  axoplasm  is  proportional  to  the 
gradient  of  the  potential  in  the  axoplasm,  i.e. 


_  -I  dV 
/"i     dx 

-_i  dV 

r,v    at   ' 


(9-1  a) 


(9-ib) 


where  h  is  the  longitudinal  current,  r,  the  axoplasm 
resistance  per  unit  length  of  axon,  V  the  potential  of 
the  axoplasm  (as  a  function  of  time,  t,  and  distance 
along  the  axon,  .v)  and  u  the  conduction  velocity.  A 
variation  in  the  longitudinal  current  with  respect  to 
space  is  associated  with  the  membrane  current,  /„, 
(Kirchoff's  law),  i.e. 

(9-2  a) 
(9-2b) 

(9-2c) 


a/i 

ax 

I  a^v 

ri  dx^ 

I    a^v 

^ 

)-i02    dt^ 

104 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


100 

-V 

r-N    Fig  23 

lm..c 

mV 

15  mm 

50 

- 

MEMBRANE 
POTENTIAL 

0 

•////////////\, 

J_ 


-100 


IMPULSE 


Fig.  24 


r\f 1 

-J   V 


V      ^ 


■^ 


''  X  0  N-//////iy/////////. 

'''/////,V.\//////^/- 


LONGITUDINAL 
CURRENT 


FIG.  23.  Diagrams  showing  tiie  spatial  and  temporal  distribution  of  the  membrane  potential,  V, 
the  longitudinal  current,  li,  and  the  membrane  current,  Im-  The  curves  for  I^  and  Im  were  obtained 
from  the  upper  curve  for  V  by  the  graphical  method  of  determining  derivatives. 

FIG.  i\.  Simultaneous  recording  of  the  membrane  action  potential  (V)  and  the  membrane  cur- 
rent (Im).  The  width  of  the  middle  pool  was  about  2  mm.  The  potential  drop  across  the  resistor  r 
was  taken  as  the  measure  of  the  meinbrane  current.  Temperature,  20°C. 


These  equations  show  that  the  membrane  current  of 
a  uniform  axon  is  proportional  to  the  second  deriva- 
tive (with  respect  to  either  time  or  space)  of  the  mem- 
brane action  potential  [cf.  Katz  &  Schmitt  (73)].  It 
should  be  pointed  out  in  this  connection  that  equa- 
tions C9-2)  were  derived  without  any  assumption  as  to 
the  behavior  of  the  resting  or  active  membrane.  These 
equations  fail  to  hold  only  when  the  axoplasm  dis- 
obeys Ohm's  law  or  when  the  propae;ation  of  the  im- 
pulse is  macroscopicalh'  nonuniform. 

Figure  23  shows  the  space  and  time  patterns  of  the 
membrane  potential,  the  longitudinal  current  and  the 
membrane  current  as  calculated  by  equations  (9-1) 
and  (9-2).  To  emphasize  the  similarity  between  the 
space  pattern  and  the  time  course  of  the  action  po- 
tential, the  impulse  is  assumed  in  this  figure  to  travel 
from  the  right-hand  end  of  the  axon  to  the  left.  The 
resistance  r;  is  assumed  to  be  1.5X10^  ohm  per  cm 
[cf.  Schmitt  (106)]  and  the  velocity  to  be  15  m  per  sec. 
It  is  seen  that  the  longitudinal  current  is  diphasic  and 
the  membrane  current  is  triphasic.  It  is  simple  to 
prove  that  the  total  area  under  the  curve  for  the  longi- 


tudinal current  or  under  the  curve  for  the  memijrane 
current  has  to  be  equal  to  zero. 

The  upper  part  of  figure  24  shows  an  approximate 
method  of  recording  the  membrane  current  of  the 
giant  axon  of  the  squid.  A  giant  axon  is  mounted 
across  three  pools  of  sea  water  separated  by  two  nar- 
row partitions.  The  large  lateral  pools  are  directly 
grounded,  and  the  small  middle  pool  is  grounded 
through  a  small  resistor.  The  membrane  current  flow- 
ing through  the  portion  of  the  fiber  in  the  middle 
pool  is  measured  by  amplifying  a  small  potential  drop 
across  the  resistor  between  the  middle  pool  and 
ground.  In  order  to  obtain  a  simultaneous  recording  of 
the  membrane  action  potential,  a  microelectrode  is 
inserted  into  the  portion  of  the  axon  in  the  middle 
pool.  The  axon  is  excited  by  a  shock  applied  near  its 
end.  The  record  presented  in  the  figure  shows  that 
the  temporal  relation  between  the  action  potential 
and  the  membrane  current  is  very  similar  to  what  has 
been  expected  from  the  results  of  the  calculations  in 
figure  23. 

W^e  shall  now  discuss  the  field  of  potential  in  the 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


surrounding  fluid  medium  produced  by  the  triphasic 
membrane  current  just  mentioned.  If  the  space-time 
pattern  of  the  membrane  current  is  given,  the  problem 
of  finding  the  potential  field  in  a  volume  conductor  is 
a  purely  physical  problem,  namely,  an  application  of 
Ohm's  law  to  the  electrolytic  conductor  around  the 
axon. 

The  simplest  example  of  problems  of  this  type  is  the 
case  in  which  a  uniform  axon  is  surrounded  through- 
out its  length  by  a  conducting  fluid  of  a  uniform  thick- 
ness (fig.  25/I).  We  assume  that  the  volume  of  fluid 
is  not  so  small  as  to  modify  the  spatial  distribution  of 
the  membrane  current  di.scussed  above.  Let  s  denote 
the  resistance  per  unit  length  of  the  surrounding  fluid 
medium;  in  the  present  case,  s  «  ^i,  where  r-,  is  the 
resistance  per  unit  length  of  the  axoplasm.  We  express 
the  potential  diflerence  across  the  axon  membrane  at 
point  .V  and  time  /  explicitly  as  l^x  +  vO,  indicating 
that  the  variation  in  the  membrane  potential  travels 
leftward  at  a  constant  velocity,  v.  Similarh,  the 
longitudinal  current  and  the  membrane  current  are 
functions  of  (.v  -{-  vt). 

It  is  simple  to  show  that  the  total  current  flowing 
through  the  whole  cross  section  of  the  surrounding 
fluid  medium  at  any  point,  .v,  at  any  moment,  t,  is 
equal  and  opposite  to  the  longitudinal  current  in  the 
axon  at  the  same  x  and  /.  To  the  present  one  dimen- 
sional approximation,  the  current  in  the  medium  at 
.V  and  time  ;  is  given  by  — /i(.v  +  vt').  Denoting  the 


^1 


Xz 


_fL 


'<'^(«?<^{M(((^</fulff:::::ffffrkh 


^  ^  /  /  /y  /^//. 


y. 


X|  X 

V77\ 


//////////A 


X2 


!>i>;'Hi>!>^!>;'; 


//^^//// 


JL 


222 


FIG.  25.  A.  A  uniform  axon  immersed  in  a  conducting  Huid 
medium  of  uniform  diameter;  the  action  potential  recorded 
with  electrodes  at  xi   and  x-;  is  given  by  the  equation  (9-3). 

B.  The  case  in  which  the  diameter  of  the  fluid  medium  changes 
at  x';  the  action  potential  recorded  is  given  by  equation  (9-4). 

C.  A  uniform  axon  immersed  in  a  large  volume  of  fluid;  the 
potential  near  the  axon  is  given  by  the  triphasic  curve  in  the 
diagram. 


potential  at  .vo  in  the  medium  referred  to  that  at  .vi  by 
U2-1,  it  is  found  that 


-  =  L 


si, fix  +  I'Odx 


VQx,  +  vt) V(,Xi  +  vt}. 


(9-3) 


[Note  that  the  integral  above  represents  a  summation 
of  the  IR  drops  along  the  fluid  medium  at  a  given 
moment  /.]  The  action  potential  recorded  externally 
with  electrodes  placed  at  Xi  and  X2,  U2-1,  consists  of 
two  terms,  one  representing  the  activity  at  Xi, 
F(Ar2  -f  vt),  and  the  other,  the  activity  at  xi,  F(xi  + 
vt).  The  amplitude  of  the  observed  potential  variation 
is  reduced  by  a  factor  of  j/n-  Equation  (9-3)  is  a 
mathematical  expression  of  what  is  known  as 'diphasic 
recording'  of  the  action  potential.  Because  of  the 
negative  sign  in  front  of  VQx^  -\-  vt),  it  was  believed 
that  the  surface  of  the  active  portion  of  an  axon  was 
'electrically  negative'  to  the  surface  at  rest.  It  should 
be  borne  in  mind,  however,  that,  if  the  surrounding 
inedium  is  not  uniform,  the  potential  on  the  active 
surface  is  not  always  negative  to  that  on  the  resting 
surface. 

The  next  simple  example  of  the  volume  conductor 
problems  is  the  case  in  which  the  resistance  per  unit 
length  of  the  conducting  fluid  medium  changes  at  x' 
suddenly  from  si  to  so  (fig.  25Z?).  Expressing  j-  as  a 
function  of  ,v,  it  is  found  that 


U,^i 


-f 


<.v)/,(.v  -I-  vt)  d.v 


- 1     n    ,  -   SV(^x  -I-  ;./)  ^ 


dx 


(9-4) 


_5  K(.v,  +  ,-0  -  — '  F(.v>  +  vt) 


+ 


V\x'  -I-  vt). 


[The  last  step  of  the  calculation  above  was  accom- 
plished by  integration  by  parts.]  The  right-hand  mem- 
ber of  equation  (9-4)  contains  three  terms,  the  first 
term  representing  the  activity  at  x-t,  the  second  term 
that  at  .V]  and  the  third  term  arising  from  the  activity 
at  .v'.  The  third  term  changes  its  sign,  depending  on 
whether  s-i  <  s\  or  .f-.>  >  S\;\X  vanishes  when  si  =  5\. 
If  Si  is  nearly  zero,  i.e.  if  the  amount  of  fluid  around 
the  axon  is  very  large  on  one  side  of  x' ,  the  second 
term  in  equation  (9-4)  vanishes  and  the  equation  in- 
dicates that  the  electrode  at  .vi  effectively  records  the 
potential  variation  at  x' . 


io6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


The  final  case  to  be  discussed  is  the  potential  field 
produced  by  a  uniform  axon  suspended  in  a  large 
volume  of  conducting  fluid.  In  this  case,  the  potential 
in  the  fluid  at  a  great  distance  away  from  the  axon  is 
not  influenced  by  the  nerve  impulse;  therefore,  the 
electrode  on  such  a  point  is  truly  "indifferent'.  Under 
such  circumstances,  the  potential  in  the  space  is  in- 
vensely  proportional  to  the  distance  from  the  source 
of  current.  Since  there  is  a  line  source  in  the  present 
case,  the  potential  at  point  P  in  the  fluid  medium  is 
given  by 


f/. 


4^  J 


d.v, 


where  .S'  is  the  specific  resistance  of  the  fluid  medium 
and  /?,,(.v)  is  the  distance  between  point  P  and  point  x. 
If  point  P  is  on  the  surface  of  the  axon  (at  x  =  p), 

U,.   cc   /,„(/)  +  ,.t\ 

since  the  source  in  the  immediate  neighborhood  ol  the 
recording  electrode  is  expected  to  have  an  over- 
whelmingly large  effect  in  determining  U,,.  The  time 
course  of  Up  is  now  triphasic  as  is  the  time  course  of 
the  membrane  current  in  figures  23  and  24.  Under 
these  circumstances  it  is  incorrect  to  say  that  the  sur- 
face of  the  active  region  of  the  axon  is  'electrically 
negative'. 

More  complicated  cases  of  the  volume  conductor 
problems  can  be  solved  by  finding  the  solution  of 
Laplace's  equation  AT'  =  o  under  the  boundary  con- 
dition described  roughly  by  ( —  i /.S)  (3  r/c)«)  = 
/,„(.v  +  r/),  where  n  is  the  normal  to  the  surface  of  the 
axon.  To  apply  this  concept  of  volume  conductors  to 
the  potential  field  in  the  body,  one  has  to  consider 
both  the  nonuniformity  of  the  excitable  tissues  and 
the  nonhomogeneity  of  the  conducting  medium.  The 
arguments  described  above  on  the  potential  field 
cau.sed  by  nerve  impulses  are  based  on  the  work  of 
Craib  (21),  Marmont  (83),  Lorente  de  No  (77), 
Tasaki  &  Takeuchi  (136)  and  others. 


NERVOUS     CONDUCTION     IN     MYELINATED     NERVE 
FIBER    (SALT.ATORY    CONDUCTION) 

The  mode  of  propagation  of  a  nerve  impulse  in 
the  vertebrate  myelinated  nerve  fil)er  is  expected  to 
be  somewhat  diflferent  from  that  in  the  invertebrate 
nerve  fiber  because  of  the  structural  discontinuities 
along  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber.  We  have  seen  that 
the  myelin  sheath  of  the  vertebrate  nerve  fiber  shows 
an  cxtremelv  hia;h  electric  resistance  to  a  direct  cur- 


rent (p.  87).  We  have  also  become  acquainted  with 
the  experimental  evidence  indicating  that  the  elec- 
tric response  of  the  nerve  fiber  derives  from  physio- 
logical activity  localized  at  nodes  of  Ranvier  of  the 
fiber  (p.  88).  The  myelinated  nerve  fiber  has  a 
cable  structure;  when  one  of  the  nodes  of  the  fiber  is 
thrown  into  action,  there  is  a  local  current  which 
tends  to  raise  the  membrane  potential  of  the  adjacent 
node  to  a  level  higher  than  the  threshold  potential. 
When  all  the  nodes  of  the  fiber  are  excitable,  there- 
fore, it  is  expected  that  the  activity  will  spread  from 
node  to  node  indefinitely  along  the  fiber.  We  shall 
examine  the  line  of  evidence  indicating  that  this  is 
actually  the  mode  of  nervous  conduction  in  the 
mvlinated   nerve  fiber. 


Effect  of  Increase  of  External  Resistance 

It  is  a  fairly  difficult  problem  to  demonstrate  that 
an  increase  in  the  resistance  of  the  external  fluid 
medium  does  affect  propagation  of  a  nerve  impulse 
in  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber.  The  reason  is  that  the 
resistance  per  unit  length  of  the  axis  cylinder  is  very 
high  (150  to  250  Mfl  per  cm)  even  in  the  largest 
nerve  fiber  in  the  frog  .sciatic  nerve.  Unless  the  ex- 
ternal resistance  is  raised  above  this  level  of  the  in- 
ternal resistance,  it  would  not  be  possible  to  demon- 
strate a  clear  effect  upon  the  process  of  nervous 
conduction. 

The  first  piece  of  evidence  along  this  line  was  ob- 
tained in  the  nerve  fiber  of  which  a  portion  was 
rendered  inexcitable  by  narcosis  (117,  135).  The 
upper  part  of  figure  26  shows  the  experimental 
arrangement  employed.  An  isolated  nerve  fiber  of 
the  toad  is  mounted  across  three  pools  of  Ringer's 
fluid  separated  by  two  narrow  air-gap  partitions.  A 
portion  of  the  fiijer,  including  two  nodes  of  Ranvier, 
is  introduced  into  the  small  middle  pool,  and  the 
remaining  portions  of  the  fiber  are  immersed  in  the 
large  lateral  pools.  In  each  of  the  three  pools,  an 
electrode  of  Ag-AgCl  Ringer  (agar)  type  is  im- 
mersed. The  electrode  in  one  of  the  lateral  pools  is 
connected  to  a  low  input  amplifier,  and  the  remaining 
two  electrodes  are  grounded. 

With  all  three  pools  filled  with  normal  Ringer's 
solution,  the  nerve  impulse  arising  at  E  in  the  figure 
alwavs  travels  across  the  two  narrow  partitions  (record 
A).  When  the  portion  of  the  fiber  in  the  middle  pool 
is  treated  with  a  cocaine-Ringer's  solution  (0.2  per 
cent),  the  impulse  fails  in  some  preparations  to 
propagate  beyond  the  narcotized  region  (record  B). 
When    the    electrode    in    the    small    middle    pool    is 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE  1 07 

Fig.  28 


y  AMP. 


'MX^^ 


Fig.26 


v-r>. 


FIG.  26.  Demonstration  of  the  dependence  of  ncivous  conduction  upon  the  flow  of  electric  current 
outside  the  fiber.  A.  Action  current  recorded  with  an  amphfier  connected  between  the  middle  and 
the  distal  pools;  stimulus  given  at  E.  B.  Block  of  conduction  caused  by  replacing  the  Huid  in  the 
middle  pool  with  an  0.2  per  cent  cocaine-Ringer's  solution.  C.  Restoration  of  conduction  by  lifting 
the  middle  electrode  from  the  surface  of  the  fluid.  Time  marks,  i  msec,  apart.  [From  Tasaki  (123).] 

FIG.  27.  Demonstration  of  the  effect  of  a  shunting  resistance  of  20  megohms  across  the  insulated 
internode  upon  nervous  conduction.  AMP  represents  a  high  input-impedance  preamplifier.  Record 
A  was  taken  with  the  resistance  disconnected;  Record  B  with  the  resistor  connected.  [From  Tasaki  & 
Frank  (128).] 

FIG.  28.  Measurement  of  the  safety  factor  in  nervous  conduction  by  narcosis.  Top  record:  Normal 
binodal  action  current.  Second  through  Joiirth  records:  3,  7,  38  and  38.1  minutes  after  introduction  of  a  3 
per  cent  urethane-Ringer's  solution  into  the  proximal  pool.  [From  Tasaki  (124).] 


lifted  above  the  surface  of  the  saline  at  this  moment, 
there  occurs  a  marked  increase  in  the  recorded  cur- 
rent and,  at  the  same  time,  the  tiine  course  of  the 
current  becomes  diphasic  (record  C).  In  a  motor 
nerve  fiber  with  its  innervating;  muscle  left  intact,  it 
is  seen  that  the  diphasicity  in  the  recorded  current  is 
always  associated  with  propagation  of  an  impulse 
across  the  narcotized  region  in  the  middle  pool. 

The  mechanism  of  restoration  of  conduction  in 
this  experiment  is  as  follows.  The  portion  of  the  fiber 
in  the  middle  pool  treated  with  cocaine  is  inexcitable. 
The  activity  of  the  portion  of  the  fiber  in  the  lateral 
pool  induces  a  current  that  spreads  along  the  fiber  in 
the  middle  pool,   but   this  spreading  current   is  too 


weak  to  e.xcite  the  portion  of  the  fiber  beyond  the 
middle  pool.  When  the  electrode  in  the  middle  pool 
is  removed,  the  leakage  of  the  spreading  current 
through  the  portion  of  the  fiber  in  the  middle  pool 
is  reduced  and,  consequently,  the  current  that  reaches 
the  other  side  of  the  middle  pool  is  increased.  Thus, 
the  spreading  current  becomes  suprathreshold  for 
the  portion  of  the  fiber  beyond  the  middle  pool. 

The  question  has  been  raised  (37,  66,  128,  145)  as 
to  whether  it  is  possible  to  block  propagation  of  a 
nerve  impulse  by  insulating  a  nerve  fiber  between 
the  two  neighboring  nodes.  First,  we  must  discuss  a 
troublesome  factor  related  to  the  experiment  de- 
signed to  answer  this  question. 


io8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


In  order  to  detect  propagation  of  ner\e  impulses 
across  an  insulating  air  gap,  it  is  necessary  to  have 
an  amplifier  or  the  innervated  muscle  attached  to 
the  single  fiber  preparation.  Stimulating  electrodes 
and  a  muscle  or  recording  electrodes  connected  to 
the  two  sides  of  the  insulating  gap  introduced  an 
electric  capacity  which,  under  ordinary  experimental 
conditions,  is  large  enough  to  establish  a  local  circuit 
(by  this  capacitativc  pathway).  The  resistance  of  a 
single  fiber  preparation  mounted  across  a  wide  air 
gap  is  of  the  order  of  50  MfJ.  If  there  is  a  capacity  of 
about  2  MMf  between  the  two  portions  across  the  gap, 
the  local  circuit  between  the  two  portions  of  the 
preparation  will  be  very  effectively  closed  by  the 
capacitative  pathway  for  a  period  of  about  o. i  msec. 
In  fresh  single  fiber  preparations,  it  is  actually  im- 
possible to  demonstrate  a  conduction  block  at  an 
insulating  air  gap  if  muscular  contractions  are  taken 
as  an  index  of  such  conduction''  (128,  145). 

The  capacitative  coupling  between  the  two  insu- 
lated portions  of  a  single  fiber  preparation  can  be 
markedly  reduced  by  the  use  of  a  positive  feed-back 
amplifier.  In  the  diagram  of  figure  27  the  small  por- 
tion of  the  preparation  on  one  side  of  the  insulating 
air  gap  is  connected  to  the  input  of  a  unity-gain 
preamplifier  and  is  completely  enclosed  in  a  metallic 
shield  driven  by  the  output  of  the  preamplifier.  (Note 
that,  when  the  potential  of  the  insulated  portion 
rises  above  the  ground  potential,  the  potential  of  the 
shield  around  the  fiber  rises  to  the  same  extent  and, 
consequently,  no  electric  charge  is  induced  between 
the  insulated  portion  of  the  preparation  and  ground. 
The  input  impedance  of  the  preamplifier  can  l)e 
made  as  high  as  1000  MQ.) 

It  is  surprising  to  see  that  most  single-fiber  prepa- 
rations mounted  as  shown  in  this  figure  are  still 
capable  of  carrying  impuLses  across  the  air  gap  (128). 
Washing  the  surface  of  the  internode  in  the  gap  with 
a  nonelectrolyte  solution  does  not  generally  help  to 
bring  about  a  block  at  the  insulating  air  gap.  Prob- 
ably, the  cell  of  Schwann  on  the  surface  of  the  nerve 
fiber  does  not  permit  us  to  raise  the  external  resist- 

'  There  are  somewhat  controversial  viewpoints  on  this 
subject  in  the  literature.  Huxley  &  Stampfli  (66)  reported 
that  conduction  was  blocked  when  the  external  resistance  was 
raised.  Wolfgram  &  van  Harreveld  (145)  failed  to  demonstrate 
a  block  under  similar  experimental  conditions  and  expressed 
the  view  that  their  experimental  results  were  inconsistent 
with  the  concept  of  saltatory  conduction.  Frankenhauser  & 
Schneider  (37)  reported  that  they  could  demonstrate  a  block 
with  a  20  MSJ  shunting  resistance  across  the  insulating  air 
gap.  For  a  further  discussion  on  this  point,  see  Tasaki  &  Frank 
(128). 


ance  high  enough  to  cause  a  conduction  block  in 
fresh  preparations. 

Record  .-1  in  figure  27  was  obtained  after  circulat- 
ing dry  air  around  the  portion  of  the  fiber  on  the  air 
gap  for  a  short  period  of  time.  This  causes  a  rapid 
e\aporation  of  water  from  the  surface  of  the  fiber 
followed  by  a  slow  desiccation  of  the  axis  cylinder. 
The  monophasicity  of  the  response  indicates  that  the 
block  has  actually  taken  place.  Record  B  in  the 
figure  was  taken  while  the  small  insulated  portion 
of  the  preparation  was  grounded  through  the  20  M12 
resistor  in  the  figure.  The  response  is  now  diphasic 
(or  rather  binodal),  indicating  that  conduction  was 
restored  by  the  shunting  resistance.  A  similar  revers- 
ible restoration  of  conduction  can  be  obtained  by  re- 
ducing the  feed-i)ack  voltage  to  the  driven  shield, 
thereijy  increasing  the  capacity  of  the  insulated  por- 
tion of  the  preparation  to  ground. 

The  obsersation  just  described  indicates  that  the 
abilit\-  of  the  ner\e  impulse  to  excite  the  adjacent 
resting  region  is  \cry  large.  As  a  consequence,  a  re- 
versible conduction  l:)lock  by  increasing  the  external 
resistance  has  been  demonstrated  so  far  in  prepara- 
tions with  a  somewhat  reduced  safety  margin.  How- 
ever, it  seems  safe  to  conclude  from  the  observations 
described  above  that  ner\-ous  conduction  in  the 
myelinated  nerve  fiber  does  depend  on  the  electric 
pathway  outside  the  myelin  sheath. 

Safety  Factor 

The  safety  factor  in  ner\ous  conduction  inay  be 
defined  as  the  ratio  of  the  action  current  of  the  nerve 
fiber  to  the  minimum  current  intensit\  necessary  for 
ner\ous  condtiction.  If  an  action  current  generated 
at  one  point  of  the  nerve  fiber  acts  as  an  electric 
stimulus  to  the  adjacent  point,  it  should  be  po.ssible 
to  measure  the  action  current  in  terms  of  the  normal 
threshold. 

The  first  attempt  to  determine  the  safety  factor  was 
made  bv  using  a  dilute  narcotic  solution  to  reduce 
the  action  current  from  one  portion  of  a  nerve  fiber 
and  by  measuring  the  minimum  intensity  of  the  cur- 
rent necessary  to  excite  the  adjacent  portion  of  the 
fiber  (135).  In  the  uppermost  part  of  figure  28  is 
shown  the  experimental  setup  used.  A  motor  nerve 
fiber  of  the  toad  is  mounted  across  two  pools  of 
Ringer's  fluid  separated  by  a  narrow  air  gap.  The 
muscle  innervated  by  the  fiber  is  left  uncut,  and 
twitches  in  the  muscle  resulting  from  stimulation  of 
the  fiber  near  its  proximal  end  are  taken  as  an  index 
of  nervous  conduction.  An  ohmic  resistor  (of  about 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


109 


0.2  M12)  is  connected  between  the  two  electrodes  im- 
mersed in  the  pools,  this  resistor  serves  to  close  the 
external  pathway  of  the  local  circuit  and  also  to 
measure  the  lons^itudinal  current  flowint^  through  the 
axis  cylinder  bridging  the  air  gap. 

When  the  two  pools  are  filled  with  normal  Ringer's 
solution,  a  familiar  action  current  which  we  often 
refer  to  as  a  '  binodal'  action  current  is  recorded. 
Based  upon  the  arguments  described  on  earlier  pages 
(p.  88),  this  action  current  is  explained  as  deriving 
mainly  from  activity  at  the  nodes  (Ni  and  No  in  the 
figure)  in  the  immediate  neighborhood  of  the  record- 
ing partition.  The  rapid  rising  phase  of  the  action 
potential  at  Ni  develops  a  large  gradient  of  potential 
along  the  axis  cylinder  between  node  Ni  and  N2;  the 
phase  of  a  strong  (2  to  3  times  lo""  amp.)  current 
flow  in  the  binodal  action  current  is  the  period  during 
which  Ni  is  active  but  N2  is  still  inactive.  When  the 
action  potential  starts  also  at  node  N>,  the  potential 
gradient  along  the  axis  cylinder  is  greatly  diminished, 
resulting  in  a  sudden  fall  in  the  longitudinal  current 
between  Ni  and  N-j.  At  the  end  of  the  action  potential 
of  a  single  node  (fig.  16),  the  membrane  potential 
falls  very  rapidly.  The  abrupt  end  in  the  binodal 
action  current  is  related  to  the  difference  in  the  time 
of  termination  of  the  action  potential  at  Ni  and  N2. 
Because  of  the  capacities  of  the  nodal  membrane  and 
of  the  myelin  sheath,  the  spread  of  current  from  No 
to  the  internode  between  Ni  and  N2  prior  to  the 
start  of  activity  at  Ni  is  very  small. 

When  a  urethane-Ringer's  solution  barely  strong 
enough  to  block  nervous  conduction  is  introduced 
into  the  proximal  pool  (in  which  Nj  is  immersed),  the 
upward  deflection  in  the  record  (representing  posi- 
tivity  of  the  right-hand  electrode  in  the  diagrain) 
gradually  decreases,  indicating  that  the  current  arising 
at  Ni  (partly  from  No)  is  reduced  b)'  narcosis.  When 
the  upward  deflection  is  reduced  to  one-fifth  to  one- 
seventh  of  the  original  size,  the  downward  deflection 
which  has  gradually  increased  during  narcosis  sud- 
denly drops  out  and,  simultaneously,  conduction 
across  the  recording  internode  fails  (the  lowermost 
record  in  fig.  28).  From  these  observations,  it  is 
found  that  the  safety  factor  is  between  fi\e  and  seven 
in  large  myelinated  nerve  fibers  of  the  toad. 

The  safety  factor  can  be  estimated  from  the  meas- 
urement of  the  threshold  membrane  potential  and 
the  nodal  action  potential.  It  has  been  shown  that 
the  action  potential  of  a  normal  node  is  approximately 
1 10  mv  at  the  peak.  When  a  membrane  potential  of 
this  size  is  developed  at  node  Ni,  the  adjacent  node 
N2  is  subjected  to  a  strong  outward  current  which 


would  raise  the  membrane  potential  by  50  to  60  mv 
if  N2  had  been  made  ine.xcitable  (124).  Since  the 
threshold  depolarization  of  a  fresh  node  is  10  to  15 
mv,  it  is  found  that  the  safety  factor  estimated  by  this 
method  is  about  five.  There  are  other  methods  of 
estimating  the  safety  factor  (124).  They  all  give  a 
figure  between  four  and  seven. 

As  the  result  of  this  large  safety  factor  in  nervous 
conduction,  a  nerve  impulse  can  travel  across  one  or 
sometimes  two  completely  narcotized  nodes  (124). 
In  the  experiment  of  figure  26  it  is  often  seen  that 
conduction  across  the  middle  pool  remains  unsus- 
pended  after  introduction  of  a  strong  narcotic  solu- 
tion. A  nerve  impulse  cannot  travel  across  three 
inexcitable  nodes. 


Dots  the  .\ervc  Imjnihc  Jiiinji  Jrom  A'odr  to  Node? 

In  1925  Lillie  (75)  found  that,  when  his  iron  wire 
model  of  a  nerve  was  covered  with  glass  tubing  broken 
at  regular  intervals,  the  activation  process  jumped 
from  one  break  to  the  next.  On  the  basis  of  this  ob- 
servation, he  pointed  out  the  possibility  that  the 
nerve  impul.se  in  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber  may 
jump  from  node  to  node  as  in  the  model.  This  model 
of  'saltatory  conduction'  has  the  following  two  fea- 
tures: (a)  the  electrochemical  changes  underlying  the 
process  of  'conduction'  are  localized  at  the  'nodes' 
and  (i)  the  time  required  for  the  conduction  of  the 
impulse  is  determined  solely  by  the  rapidity  of  the 
process  at  the  node.  In  the  model,  therefore,  the  role 
of  the  internodal  segment  is  simply  to  provide  an 
ohmic  conductance  to  the  local  circuit. 

We  have  described  the  main  line  of  evidence  indi- 
cating that,  in  the  vertebrate  myelinated  nerve  fiber, 
the  physiological  process  responsible  for  producing 
action  potentials  is  localized  at  the  nodes.  We  have 
also  seen  that,  although  the  d.c.  resistance  of  the 
myelin  sheath  is  very  high,  the  capacity  of  the  myelin 
sheath  is  large  enough  to  have  a  marked  effect  upon 
the  threshold  of  the  nerve  fiber  measured  with  short 
current  pulses  (p.  99).  This  capacity  of  the  myelin 
sheath,  therefore,  sets  a  certain  limitation  to  the 
analogy  between  propagation  of  the  activation  wave 
in  the  iron-wire  model  and  the  actual  process  of 
ner\'ous  conduction  in  the  mvelinated  nerse  filjcr. 

The  upper  part  of  figure  29.I  illustrates  the  arrange- 
ment to  demonstrate  saltatory  conduction  in  the 
model  nerve  fiber.  An  iron  wire  covered  \vith  glass 
tubings  except  at  the  '  nodes'  is  immersed  in  a  bath 
of  nitric  acid.  When  the  wire  in  the  passive  state  is 
stimulated  at  one  end,   the  process  of  activation  as 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


B 


^/  //////////////J^''//S^/////,         j-LL. 


'////'' 


v^y-ri^ 


Iz 


I  I  a/. 


r  -► 


4  Ql2  mite 


FIG.  29.  A.  Time  courses  of  the  longitudinal  current  at  two  points  in  one  internode  of  Lillie's  salta- 
tory nerve  model.  [From  Franck  (35).]  B.  Time  courses  of  the  longitudinal  current  recorded  at  two 
extreme  ends  in  one  internode  of  a  frog  nerve  fiber.  Stimulus  at  E.  [From  Hodler  ei  al.  (64).] 


recognized  by  color  changes  and  bubbling  on  the 
surface  spreads  from  node  to  node.  The  trace  repro- 
duced in  the  figure  is  the  time  course  of  the  longitudi- 
nal current  taken  from  a  recent  article  by  Franck  (35). 
Since  the  glass  tubing  is  a  perfect  insulator  of  elec- 
tricity, the  time  courses  of  the  longitudinal  currents 
recorded  at  two  different  points  in  one  internode  are 
undoubtedly  the  same. 

Figure  29^  shows  a  corresponding  observation  on 
the  real  myelinated  nerve  fiber.  By  the  arrangement 
illustrated  at  the  top,  the  longitudinal  current  is  re- 
corded at  two  points  in  one  internodal  segment.  As 
can  be  seen  in  the  tracing  below,  there  is  a  large  dif- 
ference between  the  longitudinal  currents  recorded 
at  two  points  which  are  about  1.5  mm  apart  in  this 
case.  The  difference  between  the  two  longitudinal 
currents  represents  the  double  peaked  membrane  cur- 
rent recorded  through  the  myelin  sheath  (fig.  11  A). 

We  see  in  figure  2(jB  that  the  two  longitudinal 
currents  recorded  at  two  different  points  in  one 
internode  rise  at  different  rates,  reach  the  peaks  at 
different  moments  and  fall  at  different  rates.  This  is 
a  direct  consequence  of  the  existence  of  a  large  capaci- 
tative  flow  of  current  through  the  myelin  sheath. 
Like  a  signal  travelling  along  a  submarine  cable,  the 
longitudinal  current  spreads  along  the  axis  cylinder 
at  a  finite  rate.'"  Because  of  this  slow  spread  of  the 
membrane  potential  (cf.   p.    100)  and   of  the  longi- 

'"  A  different  viewpoint  is  stated  in  a  previous  paper  by 
Huxley  &  Stampfii  (66).  The  slight  difference  between 
their  experimental  results  and  the  results  described  in  the  text 
is  probably  due  to  their  use  of  a  high  input  resistance  in  their 
amplifier  which  tends  to  lower  the  time  resolution  in  recording 
[cf.  footnote  on  p.  11,  Tasaki  (124)]. 


tudinal  current  along  the  internode,  it  is  not  legiti- 
mate to  state  that  a  nerve  impulse  jumps  from  node 
to  node  without  spending  any  time  in  the  internode. 
This  point  has  been  stressed  in  an  article  by  Hodler 
et  al.  (64)  [cf.  also  Stampfli  (i  14)].  It  has  been  pointed 
out  (125)  that  the  major  portion  of  the  temperature 
dependence  of  the  conduction  velocity  (Qio  of  about 
1.8)  can  ije  attributed  mainly  to  a  change  in  the 
cable  properties  of  the  nerve  fiber  [cf.  also  Schmitt 
C106)]. 

Field  of  Piitential  Produced  by  a  .\enr  Impulse 

We  have  discussed  in  the  preceding  section  the 
field  of  potential  produced  in  the  surrounding  fluid 
medium  by  a  nerve  impulse  travelling  along  a  uni- 
form invertebrate  nerve  fiber.  Because  of  the  struc- 
tural discontinuities  along  the  myelinated  nerve 
fiber,  the  statements  made  in  the  preceding  section 
are  not  in  a  strict  sense  applicable  to  the  myelinated 
nerve  fiber.  However,  there  is  a  special  case  in  which 
the  effect  of  the  discontinuities  is  very  small. 

Let  us  consider  the  case  in  which  a  single  nerve 
fiber  of  a  uniform  diameter  is  enclosed  in  a  glass 
tubing  of  a  uniform  diameter  filled  with  Ringer's 
.solution  (as  in  fig.  25.-1).  In  this  ca.se,  the  longitudinal 
current  at  one  point  along  the  fiber  is  equal  in  in- 
tensity and  opposite  in  sign  to  the  current  flowing 
through  the  medium  at  the  same  point.  From  the 
argument  described  in  the  preceding  section,  it  is 
found  that  the  spatial  distribution  of  the  potential 
along  the  fluid  medium  in  the  glass  tubing  is  a  mir- 
ror image  of  the  potential  inside  the  axis  cylinder, 
its  absolute  value  being  determined  by  the  ratio  of 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE  I  I  I 


the  resistance  per  unit  length  of  tlie  outside  fluid  to 
that  of  the  axis  cylinder  (equation  9-3).  This  field  of 
potential  travels  along  the  fiber  at  the  average  ve- 
locity of  the  impulse.  Insofar  as  one  disregards  the 
variations  in  the  potential  that  occur  within  one 
internodal  distance  (about  2  mm)  or  within  one 
internodal  conduction  time  (about  o.  i  msec),  the 
potential  field  produced  by  a  myelinated  nerve  fiber 
in  the  fluid  medium  is  similar  to  that  produced  by  a 
uniform  invertebrate  axon. 

The  distribution  of  the  potential  on  the  surface  of 
a  uniform  nerve  trunk  produced  by  a  nerve  impulse 
travelling  along  a  single  nerve  fiber  in  the  trunk  can 
be  regarded  as  analogous  to  the  case  described  above. 
To  the  approximation  that  the  potential  variations 
within  0.1  msec,  are  disregarded,  therefore,  the 
principle  of 'diphasic  recording  of  the  action  poten- 
tial' described  in  the  preceding  section  is  applicable 
to  this  case.  A  further  discussion  on  this  problem  can 
be  found  elsewhere  (124).  Frankenhauser  (36), 
Hodler  el  al.  (64),  Stampfli  &  Zotterman  (i  15)  and 
others  have  investigated  the  details  of  the  potential 
variations  occurring  within  one  internodal  conduction 
time  and  also  within  one  internodal  length. 

When  a  myelinated  nerve  fiber  is  immersed  in  a 
two-dimensional  or  three-dimensional  volume  con- 
ductor, the  potential  field  produced  ijy  a  nerve  im- 
pulse is  very  different  from  the  field  produced  by  an 
impulse  of  a  uniform  invertebrate  axon.  As  has  been 
shown  in  figure  1 1,  strong  sinks  of  electric  current  are 
localized  at  the  nodes  while  the  sources  are  distributed 
along  the  internodes  as  well  as  at  the  nodes.  There- 
fore, the  time  course  of  the  potential  picked  up  by  a 
recording  electrode  placed  near  one  of  the  nodes  is 
expected  to  be  very  different  from  the  record  ob- 
tained with  the  electrode  on  the  myelin  co\ered  por- 
tion of  the  fiber. 

Figure  30  shows  the  time  courses  of  the  action 
potentials  recorded  with  a  metal  microelectrode 
placed  at  various  points  near  a  node  of  Ranvier  of 
an  isolated  single  nerve  fiber  immersed  in  a  thin 
layer  of  Ringer's  solution.  The  vertical  straight  line 
in  the  middle  of  the  figure  represents  the  course  of 
the  fiber,  and  the  center  of  the  two  concentric  circles 
represents  the  position  of  the  node  under  study.  It  is 
seen  in  the  figure  that  the  largest  negative  potential 
is  observed  when  the  recording  electrode  is  placed  in 
the  immediate  neighborhood  of  the  node.  The  ampli- 
tude of  the  negative  component  of  the  action  poten- 
tial decreases  as  the  distance  from  the  node  increases, 
and  this  decrease  is  roughly  independent  of  the  direc- 
tion in  which  the  electrode  is  moved  awav  from  the 


FIG.  30.  Records  of  action  potentials  taken  with  a  small 
metal  electrode  placed  around  a  node  of  Ranvier.  The  nerve 
fiber  was  immersed  in  a  shallow  layer  of  Ringer's  solution. 
The  vertical  line  represents  the  nerve  fiber,  and  the  center 
of  the  two  concentric  circles  the  node  under  study.  The  im- 
pulse travels  downward.  Five  records  on  the  vertical  line  were 
taken  with  the  electrode  along  the  fiber  and  slightly  to  one  side. 
Other  nodes  of  the  fiber  were  not  exposed  in  the  operated 
region  of  the  preparation.  The  conduction  distance  was  about 
45    mm.    Temperature,    2o°C.    [From    Tasaki    (137).! 


fiber.  For  further  details  of  this  experiment  see  Tasaki 

(124). 

Conduct uin  in  a  Polarized  Nerve  Fiber 

When  a  direct  current  is  applied  to  a  nerve  trunk 
through  a  pair  of  nonpolarizable  electrodes  in  con- 
tact with  its  surface,  the  portion  of  the  nerve  fiber 
near  the  anode  is  traversed  by  a  continuous  inward 
membrane  current,  and  the  region  near  the  cathode 
is  subjected  to  an  outward  membrane  current.  The 
behavior  of  the  nerve  impulse  in  such  '  polarized' 
regions  of  the  nerve  fiber  was  discussed  by  Pfliiger 
(too)  more  than  a  half  century  ago.  A  nerve  fiber 
modified  by  a  constant  current  is  said  to  be  in  an 
'electrotonic'  state.  In  order  to  understand  the  be- 
havior of  a  nerxe  impulse  in  the  nerve  fiber  under 
'electrotonus',  it  is  desirable  to  investigate  the  be- 
havior of  a  single  node  preparation  under  influence  of 
a  constant  current. 

Figure  31  shows  the  effect  of  a  passage  of  a  short 
rectangular  current  pulse  upon  the  threshold  and  the 
action  potential  of  the  single  node.  The  arrangement 
employed  is  the  same  as  that  for  figure   16  (p.  94). 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH^■SK5I,0^,Y 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  3 1 .  Effect  of  short  polarizing  current  pulses  upon  the 
action  potential  of  a  single  node  of  Ranvier.  The  arrangement 
shown  in  the  upper  part  of  fig.  16  was  used.  The  action  poten- 
tial was  initiated  by  a  short  stimulating  pulse  approximately  i 
Qejl)  and  4  msec.  (jighQ  after  the  start  of  the  polarizing  pulse. 
Voltage  calibration,  50  mv;  time  marks,  i  msec.  .\  toad  nerve 
liber  at  1 1  °C.  [From  Tasaki  (126).] 


When  a  pulse  of  outward  subthreshold  current  is 
applied  through  the  nodal  membrane,  the  potential 
inside  the  node  rises  above  the  resting  level,  resulting 
in  an  upward  deflection  in  the  record.  The  threshold 
membrane  potential  measured  during  the  period  of 
current  flow  (of  about  10  msec.)  is  nearly  identical 
with  the  level  before  the  start  of  the  subthreshold 
pulse.  In  other  words,  a  weak  additional  current, 
which  is  sufficient  to  raise  the  membrane  potential 
from  the  new  level  reached  by  application  of  the  sub- 
threshold pulse  to  the  normal  threshold  level,  re- 
leases a  full-sized  action  potential.  The  membrane 
potential  at  the  peak  of  the  action  potential  is  also 
unaff"ected  by  the  constant  current. 

When  the  polarity  of  the  constant  current  is  re- 
versed, a  stronger  additional  stimulating  pulse  is  re- 
quired to  raise  the  membrane  potential  to  the 
threshold  level.  The  membrane  potential  at  the  peak 
of  the  action  potential  is  not  affected  by  application 
of  a  constant  inward  current  of  about  10  msec, 
duration. 

In  the  experiment  just  described,  if  one  regards  the 
threshold  for  the  short  (additional)  current  pulse  as  a 
function  of  the  rectangular  polarizing  current,  one 
finds  that  the  threshold  is  lowered  by  an  outward 
polarizing  current  and  raised  by  a  current  of  opposite 
polarity.  Similarly,  if  one  measures  action  potentials 
from  the  level  immediately  before  the  delivery  of  the 
short  stimulating  pulse,  it  is  found  that  the  amplitude 
is  reduced  by  an  outward  (or  cathodally  polarizing) 
current  and  increased  iiy  an  inward  (or  anodally 
polarizing)  current.  This  is  the  direct,  or  primary 
eff'ect  of  the  polarizing  current  upon  the  threshold 
and  the  action  potential. 

A  long  polarizing  current  brings  about  a  secondary 
change    in    the    membrane.    A    strong    maintained 


cathodal  polarization  caused  an  additional  decrease 
in  the  amplitude  of  the  action  potential  (cathodal 
depression)  accompanied  by  changes  in  the  mem- 
brane conductance  and  probably  in  its  emf  The 
effect  of  a  strong  anodal  polarization  is  somewhat  ob- 
scured ijy  the  strong  stimulating  current  required  to 
raise  the  membrane  potential  up  to  the  threshold 
level.  Using  intact  sciatic  nerves,  Lorente  de  No  (77) 
made  an  extensive  investigation  on  the  changes  in 
the  membrane  potential  caused  by  long  polarizing 
currents. 

Now  let  us  disctiss  in  this  connection  the  well-known 
experiment  iiy  Erlanger  &  Blair  (28)  who  in  1934 
discovered  the  electric  sign  of  the  discontinuous  na- 
ture of  nervous  conduction  in  the  myelinated  nerve 
fiber.  They  applied  anodal  polarization  to  the  por- 
tion of  the  nerve  under  the  recording  electrode  (mono- 
phasic  lead)  and  found  that,  when  the  intensity  of 
the  polarizing  current  was  gradually  increased,  the 
configuration  of  the  action  potential  of  a  single  nerve 
fiber  in  the  nerve  underwent  a  sudden  discontinuous 
change.  Figure  32  furnishes  an  example  of  their 
record.  In  record  B  the  intensity  of  the  polarizing 
current  was  maintained  at  the  critical  level  for  the 
discontinuous  change.  The  action  potential  showed 
in  one  sweep  a  distinct  notch  in  its  rising  phase,  and 
in   the   next   .sweep   (superposed   on   the   same   film) 


FIG.  32.  Changes  in  the  configuration  of  a  monophasic 
action  potential  of  a  single  nerve  fiber  in  an  intact  nerve  trunk 
produced  by  anodal  polarization  at  the  proximal  recording 
lead.  .-1.  The  normal  spike  potential.  B.  The  spike  under  anodal 
polarization  just  strong  enough  to  block  at  the  most  accessible 
node;  two  action  potentials  superposed.  C.  Further  increase 
in  the  polarizing  current  to  the  next  critical  strength.  [From 
Erlanger  &  Blair  (28).] 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


I'3 


the  component  of  the  action  potential  above  the 
notch  dropped  out  completely. 

They  did  not  consider  this  observation  as  indicating 
the  saltatory  nature  of  nervous  conduction  in  the 
myelinated  nerve  fiber.  However,  they  correctly  ex- 
plained this  discontinuity  as  being  related  to  the 
existence  of  nodes  along  the  myelinated  nerve  fiber. 
Takeuchi  &  Tasaki  (nS)  repeated  this  observation 
on  isolated  single  nerve  fibers  and  obtained  substan- 
tially the  same  result. 

The  explanation  of  the  discontinuous  change  in 
the  single  fiber  respon.se  (fig.  32)  is  as  follows.  When 
the  threshold  membrane  current  of  the  anodaliy 
polarized  node  under  the  recording  electrode  rises 
above  the  membrane  current  caused  by  the  activity 
of  the  adjacent  node,  the  response  of  the  node  under 
study  drops  out  and  a  small  potential  variation  arising 
from  the  activity  of  the  adjacent  node  is  observed. 
A  further  discussion  on  this  subject  may  be  found 
elsewhere  (124). 

Pfliign' s  Law  of  Contraction 

The  law  of  contraction  formulated  by  Pfli'iger  (loo) 
in  1859  is  at  present  of  almost  historical  interest  only. 
To  demonstrate  this  law  one  has  to  use  a  pair  of  non- 
polarizable  electrodes,  e.g.  long  chlorided  silver  wires 
imbedded  in  2  per  cent  agar-Ringer's  gel  filled  in 
glass  tubings  or  classical  electrodes  of  the  Zn-ZnS04 
type.  A  sciatic-gastrocnemius  preparation  of  the  frog 
or  toad  is  the  standard  material  used  for  this  demon- 
stration. When  pulses  of  constant  current  (of  about 
10  sec.  duration)  are  applied  to  the  nerve  trunk 
through  the  nonpolarizable  electrodes,  one  generally 
finds  that  contractions  of  the  muscle,  if  there  are 
any,  occur  only  immediately  following  the  onset  or 
following  the  end  of  the  pulse  but  not  during  the 
period  of  constant  current  flow.  The  presence  or  ab- 
sence of  contraction  depends  upon  the  intensity  of 
the  current  and  also  upon  the  arrangement  of  the 
anode  and  the  cathode  of  the  stimulating  electrodes 
with  respect  to  the  muscle.  In  table  2  an  example  is 
presented  of  the  results  of  this  type  of  observation. 
The  symbol  -|-  indicates  the  presence  and  —  the 
absence  of  a  muscular  contraction.  The  appearance 
of  a  contraction  is  a  sign  of  arrival  of  nerve  impulses 
in  the  muscle. 

If  one  takes  nerve  impulses  carried  to  the  muscle 
by  a  single  nerve  fiber  as  an  index,  one  obtains  a 
result  somewhat  different  from  that  stipulated  by 
the  classical  law.  The  result  obtained  after  cutting 
all  but  one  fiber  near  the  mu.scle  is  also  shown  in 


T.\BLE  2.  Demonstration  of  Pfliiger' s  Law  of  Contraction 


Current 

Cathode- 

Anode-Muscle 

Anode. Cath 

ode-Muscle 

Intensity 

(Ascending) 

(Descending) 

wA 

Make 

Break 

Make 

Break 

4.5 

+  (-) 

- 

+  (-) 

— 

6 

-1- 

— 

+ 

— 

18 

-f- 

— 

-f 

— 

30 

+ 

— 

+ 

— 

.52 

+ 

-f-(±) 

-f- 

+  C-) 

75 

+  C-) 

+  (±) 

-1- 

+(-) 

98 

+(-) 

+ 

-1- 

+  C±) 

120 

+(.-') 

+  (±) 

+ 

+(±) 

144 

+(-) 

+  C±) 

+ 

+(-) 

166 

_ 

+(±) 

+ 

±(-) 

188 

— 

+(-) 

+ 

— 

This  table  indicates  the  presence,  4-,  or  the  absence,  — , 
of  a  muscular  contraction  on  make  or  break  of  long  current 
pulses  applied  to  the  nerve  trunk  of  a  sciatic-gastrocnemius 
preparation  of  the  toad.  The  orifice  of  the  electrodes  (Ag- 
AgCl  type)  was  about  6  mm  in  diameter  and  the  space  be- 
tween the  two  electrodes  was  also  about  6  mm.  The  resistance 
of  the  nerve  between  electrodes  was  approximately  10 
kilohms.  The  results  obtained  after  cutting  all  the  nerve  fibers 
near  the  muscle  except  one  large  motor  fiber  are  presented 
in  parenthesis,  and  is  mentioned  only  when  it  is  different 
from  that  for  the  whole  nerve  preparation. 


table  2.  There  are  more  negative  signs  in  this  case 
than  in  the  case  for  the  whole  nerve  trunk.  This 
difference  arises  from  the  situation  that  there  are  in 
the  nerve  trunk  many  fibers  which  are  situated  in 
different  parts  of  the  potential  field  (produced  by  the 
applied  current).  The  existence  of  the  small  motor 
nerve  fibers  which  produce  slow  muscular  contrac- 
tions (134)  in  the  nerve  trunk  makes  also  some  dif- 
ference between  a  single  fiber  and  a  nerve  trunk  ex- 
periment. 

If  one  applies  current  pulses  directly  to  the  iso- 
lated portion  of  a  single  motor  nerve  fiber  in  this 
type  of  observation,  one  finds  more  negative  signs 
than  in  the  two  previous  cases.  In  this  type  of  direct 
stimulation  of  a  single  nerve  fiber,  it  is  very  difficult 
to  demonstrate  excitation  of  the  fiber  on  break  of  an 
applied  current.  Break  excitation  which  is  readily 
observable  in  the  nerve  trunk  is  evidently  due  mainly 
to  the  capacities  of  the  myelin  sheath  and  of  the  con- 
nective tissues.  These  elements  in  the  nerve  trunk 
tend  to  generate  outward  membrane  currents  at  the 
nodes  of  the  fibers  on  withdrawal  of  the  applied  cur- 
rent. 

The  mechanism  of  anodal  block  of  nerve  conduc- 
tion has  been  discussed  on  previous  pages.  The  ab- 


114 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


senceof  a  contraction  on"  make'  of  a  strong' ascending' 
current  pulse  in  the  table  indicates  that  the  nerve 
impulse  initiated  under  the  cathode  could  not  pass 
through  the  anodally  polarized  region  of  the  nerve 
between  the  cathode  and  the  muscle. 

Effect  of  Narcosis  upon  Nervous  Conduction 

It  has  Ijeen  pointed  out  that  narcotics,  such  as 
cocaine,  urethane,  ethanol  and  others,  depress  or 
eliminate  electric  responses  of  the  nerve  fiber  when 
they  are  applied  to  the  nodes  of  Ranvier  of  the  fiber 
(p.  109).  The  action  of  these  chemicals,  as  well  as 
the  effect  of  low  sodium  in  the  medium,  progresses 
with  surprising  rapidity;  an  equilibrium  between  the 
single  fiber  and  the  surrounding  fluid  medium  con- 
taining these  chemicals  is  established  within  one 
second  (68,  71,  124). 

It  is  well  known  that  the  action  of  these  narcotics 
upon  the  whole  nerve  trunk  is  extremely  slow  and 
gradual,  as  emphasized  by  Winterstein  (144).  Evi- 
dently the  time  required  for  dififusion  of  the  chemical 
into  the  nerve  trunk  accounts  for  the  slow  action  of 
these  chemicals  upon  it.  The  diagram  in  figure  33 
illustrates  this  great  difference  in  the  rapidity  of  ac- 
tion of  urethane  between  the  intact  sciatic  nerve  and 


O  min 
O 

_J 
CD 

-:»40 


I- 
O 

q30 

Z 

o 
o 

tr 
020 

b. 

Q 
UJ 

cc. 

510 

o 

LJ 
CC 

UJ    0 


',    SCIATIC 
'  NERVE 


o 

9 


e> 
© 
o 

\ 

o 


^8.  § 


SINGLE 

NERVE    FIBER 


o      ~« 
o     o 


I 


20  2  4  6  8%    10 

^  URETHANE      RINGER 

FIG.  33.  Relation  between  the  concentration  of  urethane 
(abscissae)  and  the  time  required  for  conduction  block  (ordi- 
nates')  in  a  single  fiber  preparation  (continuous  line)  and  in  the 
whole  sciatic  nerve  (circles').  [From  Tsukagoshi,  cited  by  Tasaki 
in  (124).] 


the  exposed  nerve  fibers.  The  circles  in  the  figure 
represent  the  times  required  for  conduction  block  at 
various  concentrations  of  urethane-Ringer's  solution 
in  intact  sciatic  nerves.  The  narcotic  was  applied  to 
a  1 5  mm  long  uniform  portion  of  the  sciatic  nerve 
of  the  toad,  and  the  disappearance  of  muscular  con- 
traction was  taken  as  an  index  of  block.  The  relation- 
ship between  the  time  required  for  conduction  block 
and  the  concentration  of  urethane  is  given  by  a 
smooth  curve. 

When  a  single  nerve  fiber  preparation  or  a  few 
fiber  preparations  with  a  15  mm  long  exposed  region 
are  used  in  this  type  of  e.xperiment,  an  entirely  dif- 
ferent result  is  obtained.  For  concentrations  lower 
than  about  i  .8  per  cent,  conduction  through  the 
narcotized  region  remains  unblocked  for  an  hour  or 
more;  and  for  concentrations  higher  than  about  2.2 
per  cent,  conduction  block  sets  in  within  one  second. 
At  the  critical  concentration,  which  was  appro.xi- 
mately  2  per  cent  in  this  experiment,  conduction 
block  occurs  within  about  i  minute.  The  relationship 
between  the  concentration  and  the  time  required  for 
blocking  is,  therefore,  given  by  the  thick  line  (bending 
at  almost  a  right  angle)  in  the  figure. 

We  have  pointed  out  that  a  nerve  impulse  can 
jump  across  one  or  two  complete  inexcitable  nodes. 
If  one  reduces  the  length  of  the  narcotized  region 
down  to  about  5  mm  or  less,  a  longer  time  is  required 
to  block  conduction  since  time  must  be  allowed  for 
diffusion  of  the  narcotic  along  the  nerve.  This  fact 
was  once  taken  as  evidence  for  '  decremental  conduc- 
tion' in  the  narcotized  region  of  the  nerve  (23,  69, 
82). 

Narcotizing  solutions  of  below  the  critical  concen- 
tration applied  to  a  node  raise  the  threshold  and  lower 
the  amplitude  of  the  response.  The  magnitude  of  this 
narcotizing  effect  depends  on  the  concentration  used. 
Based  on  the  experimental  data  on  the  effects  of 
narcosis  on  single  nodes,  it  is  possible  to  explain 
nianv  phenomena  related  to  narcosis  of  the  nerve. 
The  detail  of  the  accounts  along  this  line  may  be 
found  elsewhere  (124). 


.\FTER-POTENTL^LS  .^iND  RHYTHMICAL  .\CTIVITY 

We  shall  devote  this  section  to  two  subjects  which 
are  less  clearly  understood  at  present  than  tho.se 
previously  discussed,  namely  after-potentials  and 
rhythmical  activity  of  the  nerve  fiber.  The  relation- 
ship between  after-potentials  and  rhythmical  activity 


CONDCCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


is  not  a  direct  one,  but  in  some  cases  they  are  clearly 
related  to  each  other. 

After-Piilentials 

The  term  '  after-potential'  was  introduced  by  Gasser 
and  his  associates  [cf.  Gasser  &  Erlanger  (38);  Gas- 
ser &  Graham  (39)]  to  describe  the  small,  slowly 
declinina;  potential  change  that  follows  the  large, 
short  'spike-potential'  in  the  monophasic  action  po- 
tential of  a  nerve  trunk.  The  records  furnished  in  the 
left  column  of  figure  34  show  monophasic  action 
potentials  of  the  nerve  trunk  taken  at  slow  sweep 
speeds.  The  action  potential  of  A-fibers  (top)  shows 
very  little  after-potentials,  but  the  responses  of  B- 
and  C-fibers  manifest  large  after-potentials  following 
the  sharp  spike-potentials.  These  potentials  were  re- 
corded with  extracellular  electrodes  (fig.  i)  from  three 
different  nerve  trunks  of  the  cat. 

Let  us  ne.xi  discuss  the  after-potentials  recorded 
from  single  fiber  preparations.  In  the  right-hand 
column  of  figure  34  are  shown  the  time  courses  of 
the  action  potentials  of  three  different  kinds  of  e.\- 
citable  elements.  An  upward  deflection  in  these 
records  represents  a  rise  in  the  intracellular  potential 
(referred  to  the  potential  of  the  surrounding  fluid 
medium).  The  'retention'  of  a  higher  potential  level 


Mf 


I... I.. 


.I...I...I. 


Tiiiec 


50 
msec 


200  msec 


FIG.  34.  After-potentials  in  nerve  trunks  Qejl)  and  in  single 
fibers  (right).  A.  Response  of  mammalian  .-X-fibers.  B.  Re- 
sponse of  mammalian  B-fibers.  C.  Response  of  mammalian 
C-fibers.  [The  three  records  on  the  left  are  from  Grundfest 
(43)-]  ^J'  Action  potential  of  a  toad  muscle  fiber,  recorded 
intracellularly.  Nf.  Response  of  a  toad  nerve  fiber  poisoned 
with  veratrine.  Sf.  .Action  potential  of  a  squid  giant  axon. 
Time  marks  on  the  right  in  msec. 


in  the  upper  two  records  is  often  called  a  '  negative' 
after-potential,  because  an  action  potential  was  con- 
sidered in  the  classical  physiology  as  a  "  negative' 
variation  of  the  potential  of  the  nerve  surface  (cf.  p. 
105).  Evidently,  the  term  'negative'  after-potential  is 
at  present  confusing  and  inadequate. 

The  after-potential  of  the  frog  (or  toad)  muscle 
fiber  (fig.  34,  right  top)  seems  to  decay  roughly  at 
the  time  constant  of  the  membrane  (30).  This  after- 
potential  is  not  associated  with  any  measurable  change 
in  the  membrane  resistance.  These  facts  suggest  that, 
following  one  whole  cycle  of  activity  of  the  muscle 
fiber  membrane,  there  is  an  excessive  charge  of  elec- 
tricity remaining  in  the  large  capacity  of  the  mem- 
brane and  this  charge  is  dissipated  through  the  mem- 
brane resistance.  In  the  nodal  membrane  of  the  toad 
(or  frog)  nerve  fiber,  the  time  constant  of  the  mem- 
brane is  far  shorter  than  the  duration  of  the  spike 
potential  (table  i,  p.  89);  therefore,  an  after-poten- 
tial of  this  type  does  not  exist  in  the  amphibian  nerve 
fiber. 

The  after-potential  of  the  frog  nerve  fiber  shown  in 
figure  34,  right  center,  was  induced  by  poisoning  the 
fiber  with  veratrine,  an  alkaloid  which  is  known  to 
cause  rhythmical  activity  in  the  mu.scle  and  nerve. 
Gasser  &  Graham  (39)  have  shown  that  this  chemical 
greatly  enhances  the  (negative)  after-potential  of  the 
nerve  trunk.  The  after-potential  of  this  type  is  asso- 
ciated with  a  concomitant  decrease  in  the  membrane 
resistance  (108,    133). 

The  after-potential  in  the  squid  giant  axon  i.fig.  34, 
right  bottom)  is  often  referred  to  as  an  'undershoot': 
the  membrane  potential  stays,  after  the  end  of  the 
main  spike  potential,  below  the  initial  level  of  the 
resting  potential.  As  we  have  seen  in  the  record  of 
figure  12,  this  after-potential  is  associated  with  a 
pronounced  decrease  in  the  membrane  resistance. 
Grundfest  el  al.  (45)  found  that  there  is  a  phase  of 
slightly  increased  membrane  impedance  following  the 
period  of  decreased  membrane  impedance.  In  the 
sodium  theory  (p.  118),  the  undershoot  in  the  squid 
giant  axon  is  attributed  to  an  increase  in  the  potas- 
sium permeability  of  the  membrane. 

The  nature  of  the  after-potentials  in  B-  and  C- 
fibers  in  the  vertebrate  nerve  is  not  clear.  Further 
discussions  on  the  after-potentials  of  the  nerve  trunk 
are  found  in  the  monograph  bv  Gasser  &  Erlanger 
(38). 

Rhythmical  Activitj! 

In  excitable  tissues  in  living  organisms,  action  po- 
tentials appear,  as  a  rule,  in  more-or-less  rapid  sue- 


lib 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHVSIOLOCi'    I 


cession.  Thus  motor  nerve  cells  in  the  vertebrate 
spinal  cord  discharge  impulses  repetitively  over  a 
wide  rang;e  of  frequency  depending;  on  the  state  of 
the  cell.  Similarly,  sensory  nerve  fibers  carry  a  series 
of  impulses  toward  the  spinal  cord  in  response  to 
sensory  stimuli  delivered  to  their  endings.  There  is 
at  present  a  large  amount  of  data  concerning  the 
pattern  of  impulse  discharge  obtained  by  the  method 
of  recording  single  fiber  responses  originated  by 
Adrian  (3,  4). 

In  many  excitable  tissues,  application  of  a  long 
constant  current  generates  a  train  of  action  potentials, 
as  shown  by  Arvanitaki  (8),  Fessard  (33),  Erlanger  & 
Blair  (29),  Katz  (72)  and  others.  The  records  fur- 
nished in  figure  35  show  repetitive  firing  of  action 
potentials  in  the  .squid  giant  a.xon  induced  by  con- 
stant outward  membrane  currents  of  four  different 
intensities.  The  stimulating  pulses  are  sent  into  the 
a.xon  through  a  long  intracellular  metal  wire  elec- 
trode, and  the  responses  are  recorded  with  another 
intracellular  electrode.  It  is  difficult  to  maintain 
repetitive  firing  indefinitely  under  these  experimental 
conditions.  It  is  to  be  observed  that  each  action  po- 
tential is  preceded  by  a  slowly  rising  phase  of  the 
membrane  potential.  This  slowly  rising  phase  has 
been  demonstrated  at  the  sites  of  naturally  induced 
repetitive  responses  in  the  automatically  beating 
cardiac  muscle  [cf.  VVeidmann  (143)]. 

The  site  at  which  impulses  are  initiated  repetitively 
is  called  a  'pacemaker'.  At  present,  it  is  not  clear  how- 
sensory  nerve  endings  or  the  motor  nerve  cells  become 
pacemakers.  However,  there  is  one  thing  that  can  be 
inferred  from  the  mechanism  of  the  nervous  conduc- 
tion in  the  peripheral  nerve  fiber.  As  has  been  dis- 
cussed on  previous  pages,  nervous  conduction  is  ef- 
fected through  excitation  of  each  segjment  (or  node 


of  RanvierJ  l)y  the  electric  current  generated  b\  the 
adjacent  active  segment.  From  this  one  can  infer  that 
a  sensory  stimulus  or  a  natural  stimulus  for  the  motor 
nerve  cell  has  to  be  transformed  eventually  into  an 
electric  stimulus  in  order  that  it  initiates  a  propagated 
impulse.  (If  the  size  and  shape  of  the  electric  current 
generated  by  a  sensory  stimulus  are  similar  to  those 
of  the  ordinary  action  current,  the  statement  just 
made  has  no  meaning;  however,  it  is  generally  ac- 
cepted that  the  first  electrical  sign  of  the  response  to 
a  sensory  stimulus  is  variable  in  size  and  very  differ- 
ent from  the  ordinary  all-or-none  response.)  Since  a 
constant  current  applied  to  a  peripheral  nerve  fiber 
can  gi\'e  rise  to  a  repetitive  firing  of  impulses,  it  is 
generally  believed  that  natural  pacemakers  resemble 
in  some  respect  an  artificial  one  induced  i)y  applica- 
tion of  a  constant  current  (fig.  35). 

The  mechanism  of  repetitive  firing  proposed  by 
Adrian  (3,  4)  to  interpret  the  injury  and  sensory  dis- 
charges of  impulses  is  as  follows.  An  electric  stimulus 
of  a  constant  intensity  sets  up  the  first  action  potential 
in  accordance  with  the  law  of  electric  excitation. 
Then,  the  nerve  fiber  falls  into  the  refractory  period 
which  makes  the  stimulus  totally  ineffective.  As  the 
fiber  recovers  from  this  refractoriness,  the  stimulus 
becomes  effective  again  and  the  second  action  po- 
tential is  set  up.  The  second  response  leaves  behind 
it  another  refractory  period.  The  nerve  fiber  thus 
exhibits  a  kind  of  oscillatory  phenomenon  similar  to 
that  in  a  neon  lamp  connected  to  a  battery,  a  con- 
denser and  a  resistor. 

It  is  simple  to  express  Adrian's  concept  in  terms  of 
the  membrane  potential  and  the  threshold  depolariza- 
tion. At  the  beginning  of  the  refractory  period,  the 
critical  membrane  potential  is  close  to  the  level  of 
the   shoulder   of  the   action    potential   (see   fig.    20). 


FIG.  35.  Repetitive  firing  of  action  potentials  in  a  squid  giant  axon.  The  relative  intensities  of  the 
stimulating  currents  used  are  indicated  by  the  broken  lines.  Both  stimulating  and  recording  elec- 
trodes were  long  intracellular  metal  wires.  [From  S.  Hagiwara  e!  al.,  unpublished.] 


CONDUCTIOiN    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


11/ 


During  the  relatively  refractory  period  there  is  a  con- 
tinuous recovery  in  the  threshold  membrane  potential. 
This  concept  of  Adrian  seems  to  explain  many  facts 
known  al:)out  repetitive  firing.  In  tissues  with  a  time 
constant  which  is  much  longer  than  the  duration  of 
the  action  potential,  however,  not  only  the  recovery 
process,  but  also  the  time  required  to  charge  the 
membrane  capacity  is  considered  to  influence  the 
rhythm  of  repetitive  firing  (54).  It  is  also  known  that 
the  oscillation  in  the  membrane  potential  at  sub- 
threshold levels  (8,  9)  plays  an  important  role  in 
production  of  rhythmical  activity  in  some  tissues. 

In  connection  with  the  pacemaker  mechanism, 
there  is  an  interesting  phenomenon  which  seems  to 
deserve  a  short  discussion.  That  is  'resetting'  of  the 
rhythm  of  the  repetitive  response  by  an  '  extra  im- 
pulse' reaching  the  pacemaker.  In  1936  Gilson  (41) 
examined  the  effect  of  an  artificial  (electric)  stimula- 
tion of  the  sinus  of  the  turtle  heart  upon  the  rhythm 
of  the  heart  beat.  He  found  that  the  time  interval 
between  the  artificiallv  induced  response  and  the 
following  (natural)  response  is  approximately  equal 
to  the  normal  inter\'al  of  the  automatically  induced 
responses,  regardless  of  the  interval  between  the 
artificially  induced  respon.se  and  the  preceding  one. 
Similar  phenomena  have  been  demonstrated  in 
natural  and  artificial  pacemakers  in  the  sensory 
nerve  fiber  and  in  the  motor  nerxe  fiber  [cf.  Tasaki 
(121)]. 


CURRENT    THEORIES    OF    THE    RESTING    .-^ND 
.ACTION    POTENTI.ALS 

In  the  last  section  of  this  chapter,  we  shall  briefly 
discuss  the  current  theories  dealing  with  the  mecha- 
nism whereby  the  resting  and  action  potential  of  the 
nerve  or  muscle  fiber  is  generated.  This  problem  has 
been  extensively  and  authoritatively  reviewed  by 
many  recent  inv  estigators  in  a  svmposium  Electrochemis- 
try in  Biology  and  Medicine,  edited  by  Shedlovsk\-  (l  1 1). 
The  great  variety  of  the  views  maintained  by  recent 
investigators  toward  the  present  problem  indicates 
that  the  current  theories  to  be  described  below  are 
not  yet  accepted  as  unequivocal.  We  shall  make  an 
attempt  to  explore  the  sources  of  equivocalities  and 
controversies  in  the  present  problem. 


Resting  Potential 

Twenty    years    before    the    turn    of    the    century, 
Biedermann  (12,  p.  354)  discovered  that  application 


of  an  isosmotic  potassium  chloride  solution  to  a  por- 
tion of  a  muscle  generates  a  large  potential  difference 
between  the  site  of  application  and  the  remaining 
surface  of  the  muscle.  Later,  Hober  (49)  extended 
this  observation  and  found  that  the  ability  of  various 
cations  to  affect  the  resting  potential  of  the  muscle 
increases  in  the  following  series:  Li,  Na,  Mg,  Cs, 
NH4,  Rb,  K.  Hober  found  also  that  the  correspond- 
ing series  for  anions  is  CNS,  NO3,  I,  Br,  CI,  acetate, 
HPO4,  SO 4,  tartarate. 

In  1902  Bernstein  (10)  published  the  .so-called 
'  membrane  theory'  in  which  he  postulated  a)  that 
the  resting  potential  is  pre-existent  at  the  plasma 
membrane  of  the  cell  (prior  to  injury  or  application 
oi  potassium  salts),  and  h)  that  the  resting  potential 
is  maintained  by  virtue  of  the  semipermealsility  of 
the  plasma  meinbrane.  At  that  time,  the  pre-existence 
of  ions  in  the  electrolyte  solution  (Arrhenius,  1883) 
was  known,  and  osmotic  phenomena  in  the  mem- 
brane of  some  plant  cells  and  in  artificial  membranes 
(Pfeffer,  1877)  were  also  well  understood.  Nernst's 
book  on  theoretical  chemistry  dealing  with  concentra- 
tion cells  had  just  appeared  at  that  time  (1900). 

A  present,  there  is  no  doubt  about  the  validity  of 
the  membrane  theory  in  the  form  described  above. 
There  are  in  Bernstein's  theory  two  additional  postu- 
lates. He  speculated  that  the  resting  potential  is  a  diffu- 
sion potential  resulting  from  the  difference  in  the  mo- 
bility of  potassium  and  phosphate  ions  through  the 
membrane  and  also  that  the  action  potential  is  caused 
by  a  reduction  of  the  resting  potential  resulting  from 
a  nonspecific  increa.se  of  permeability  of  the  mem- 
brane during  activity. 

Later  on,  a  large  volume  of  work  was  published 
showing  that,  within  a  certain  limit,  the  relationship 
between  the  resting  potential,  Er,  and  the  external 
potassium  concentration,  [K]o,  can  be  expressed  by 
the  Nernst  equation 


E,  =  58  los  r— r  ^'"^'^ 


(.2-0 


where  [K]i  represents  the  concentration  of  potas- 
sium in  the  protoplasm  (7,  55,  68,  76,  94).  However, 
the  validity  of  equation  (12-1)  does  not  by  itself 
prove  that  the  process  of  diffusion  of  potassium  ions 
is  responsible  for  the  resting  potential. 

Equation  (12-1)  represents  the  theoretical  maxi- 
mum (absolute)  value  of  the  resting  potential  that 
can  be  attained  if  the  concentration  gradient  of 
potassium  were  the  cause  of  the  resting  membrane 
potential.  If,  therefore,  it  happens  under  any  circum- 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Stances  that  the  observed  membrane  potential  ex- 
ceeds the  value  given  by  equation  (12-1),  one  is 
forced  to  believe  that  the  resting  potential  is  gener- 
ated primarily  by  some  electrochemical  mechanism 
other  than  the  diffusion  of  the  potassium  ion.  This 
type  of  evidence  against  the  potassium  theory  has 
been  expressed  by  several  in\estis;ators  though  not 
in  a  written  form  until  the  recent  work  of  Shaw  et  al. 

(109)- 

The  electrochemical  nature  of  the  plasma  mem- 
brane is  not  yet  clearly  understood.  Osterhout  (94), 
Beutner  (ii)  and  others  assume  that  the  resting 
potential  is  maintained  across  an  oil  (nonaqueous) 
layer.  Teorell  (139),  SoUner  (112)  and  others  have 
developed  the  concept  of  a  charged  porous  mem- 
brane as  the  site  of  bioelectric  potential.  .Shedlo\'skv 
(iio)  stressed  the  asymmetry  of  the  membrane  with 
respect  to  two  surfaces  and  the  possible  role  of  protons 
in  generation  of  the  bioelectric  potentials. 

To  explain  the  divergence  of  the  obser\ed  resting 
potential  from  the  Nernst  equation,  Hodgkin  (55) 
used  the  modified  Goldman  equation  (42).  There  is 
.some  doubt  as  to  the  applicability  of  this  equation 
to  li\ing  cells,  because  of  the  assumption  of  a  uniform 
field  (i.e.  no  charge  in  the  membrane)  adopted  in 
deriving  this  equation  (139,  p.  338).  Boyle  &  Conway 
(17)  found  that  the  ratio  of  chloride  across  the  muscle 
fiber  membrane  is  close  to  the  ratio  [K]o/[K],  and 
argued  that  the  resting  potential  of  the  skeletal 
muscle  fiber  is  a  Donnan  potential.  There  are,  how- 
ever, some  arguments  against  this  notion  (44). 

Actum  Poleiilial 

There  is  at  present  only  one  widelv  accepted 
theory  of  action  potential  production.  That  is  the 
so-called  sodium  theory  postulated  by  Hodgkin  & 
Huxley  (57,  58,  59).  Previously  Nachmansohn 
(89)  advanced  a  theory  in  which  acetylcholine  is 
assumed  to  play  a  decisive  role  in  action  potential 
production.  Recently,  however,  he  shifted  his  effort 
toward  an  attempt  to  supply  a  biochemical  basis 
for  the  sodium  theory  (90). 

This  theory  started  with  the  de\elopmcnt  of  the 
modern  technique  of  recording  and  controlling  the 
intracellular  potential.  When  it  was  found  that  the 
amplitude  of  the  membrane  action  potential  is  sub- 
stantially larger  than  the  resting  potential  across  the 
memjjrane  (p.  84),  physiologists  realized  that  Bern- 
stein's postulate  as  to  the  origin  of  the  action  potential 
(p.  117)  is  incorrect.  The  finding  of  Hodgkin  & 
Katz  (62^  that  the  amplitude  of  the  action  potential 


of  the  squid  giant  axon  varies  with  approximately 
58  mv  times  the  logarithm  of  the  concentration  of 
sodium  in  the  external  medium  (p.  93)  has  led 
the.se  British  physiologists  to  postulate  that  the  mem- 
brane potential  at  the  peak  of  acti\its-  is  determined 
by  the  concentration  gradient  of  the  .sodium  ion 
across  the  axon  membrane.  (According  to  this  pos- 
tulate, the  amplitude  of  the  action  potential  should 
vary  with  58  m\-  times  the  logarithm  of  the  intracel- 
lular concentration  of  sodium;  however,  it  is  difficult 
in  practice  to  alter  the  sodium  concentration  in  a 
wide  range.) 

Hodgkin  &  Huxley  (59)  elaborated  this  concept 
further  and  explained  the  mechanism  of  action  po- 
tential production  by  assuming  that  the  increase  in 
the  membrane  conductance  during  activity  (p.  89) 
is  a  specific  increase  of  permeaijility  to  sodium  ions. 
They  tried  to  substantiate  this  idea  by  voltage  clamp 
experiments  (p.  91).  Their  success  in  reconstruct- 
ing the  action  potential  from  the  data  obtained  by 
the  voltage  clamp  technique  is  often  regarded  as 
sufficient  proof  of  the  sodium  theory. 

The  diatjram  in  figure  36,  right,  shows  the  equiva- 
lent circuit  of  the  excitable  membrane  postulated  in 
the  theory.  When  the  membrane  is  at  rest,  the  con- 
ductance of  the  membrane  is  maintained  by  the  per- 
meabilit\'  of  the  membrane  to  potassium  ions;  i.e. 
gK  »  gsii!  where  gk  is  the  'potassium  conductance' 
and  g^a  the  'sodium  conductance'  of  the  membrane. 
This  situation  should  iiring  the  potential  of  the 
resting  membrane  close  to  E^i  which  is  defined  by 
equation  (12-1).  E^ia  in  the  diagram  represents  the 
'sodium  equilibrium  potential'  defined  ijy  the  equa- 
tion of  the  type  of  equation  (12-1)  for  the  sodium 
ion;  the  polarity  of  E^.,  is  opposite  to  that  of  Ek-  If 
g^:,  increases  at  the  peak  of  activity  to  a  \alue  well 
aboN'e  gK,  the  niemljrane  potential  should  approach 


RESTING 
+-  +  -»-  4-         

RESTING 
+  -1-  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +- 

_ 

ACTIVE 
REGION 

-1- 

+  +  +  *-^ 

FIG.  36.  Right. ■  The  equivalent  circuit  proposed  by  Hodgkin 
&  Hu.xley  to  represent  tiie  membrane  of  the  squid  giant 
axon.  Left:  The  state  of  an  axon  carrying  an  impulse  proposed 
by  the  same  authors.  The  signs  4-  and  —  indicate  the  electric 
charges  on  the  capacity  which  are  assumed  to  determine  the 
membrane  potential.  Note  that  this  concept  of  charges  on  the 
condenser  determining  the  membrane  potential  is  inapplicable 
to  the  circuit  diagram  of  fig.  9C. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


"9 


E^,,;  this  explains  the  reversal  of  the  membrane  poten- 
tial during  activity.  If  ^^a  is  increased  to  some  extent 
b\  a  stimulating  current  pulse,  a  further  increase  in 
gif.,  can  be  brought  about  by  a  regenerati\e  process; 
an  increase  in  ^^a  causes  a  rise  in  the  memijrane 
potential  which  in  turn  gives  rise  to  a  further  increase 
in  gf^i,.  The  theory  is  self-consistent.  The  readers  who 
are  interested  in  this  beautiful  scheme  are  referred  to 
the  original  article  (59). 

It  may  be  worth  pointing  out  that  there  are  in  the 
sodium  theory  a  number  of  assumptions  that  are  not 
directly  proved  by  experiments.  They  assume  in  the 
first  place  that  the  axon  membrane  under  voltage 
clamp  is  spatially  uniform;  this  may  not  be  a  safe 
assumption.  They  assume  also  that  the  capacit\-  of 
the  membrane  is  connected  in  parallel  to  the  cmf  of 
the  membrane  (p.  85).  They  did  not  exclude  the 
possibility  that  the  sodium  ions  bound  in  the  sub- 
stance of  the  membrane  (instead  of  the  free  sodium 
ions  in  the  medium)  exert  direct  influence  upon  the 
amplitude  of  the  action  potential.  There  are  several 
more  assumptions  in  the  theory.  Although  most  of 
these  assumptions  appear  to  be  reasonable,  it  is  also 
true  that  one  can  make  a  set  of  entirely  difTerent 
assumptions  and  explain  almost  the  same  amount  of 
experimental  data. 

There  is  at  present  a  large  volume  of  work  dealing 
with  the  movement  of  sodium  or  potassium  ions 
across  the  excitable  membrane.  The  principal  findings 


pertinent  to  the  discussion  in  this  chapter  are  a)  a 
steady  outward  current  through  the  axon  membrane 
is  carried  almost  exclusively  by  potassium  ions  (60), 
and  b)  there  is  an  exchange  of  intracellular  potassium 
with  extracellular  sodium  associated  with  repetitive 
excitation  of  the  axon  (74).  It  is  generally  agreed  that 
the  amount  of  the  Na-K  exchange  associated  with 
repetitive  excitation  observed  in  invertebrate  axons 
is  close  to  the  \alue  expected  from  the  sodium  theory. 
It  should  Ije  kept  in  mind  in  this  connection  that 
there  are  excitable  tissues  which  do  not  require  any 
sodium  ion  in  the  medium  to  produce  action  potentials. 
Crustacean  muscles  studied  by  Fatt  &  Katz  (31) 
are  a  well-known  example,  and  the  plant  cell,  Nilella, 
investigated  by  Osterhout  and  his  associate  (93,  94) 
is  another.  This  fact  suggests  that  the  role  of  the 
sodium  ion  in  the  medium  inight  be  only  indirectly 
connected  with  the  process  of  action  potential  pro- 
duction. The  alternatis'e  explanation  of  this  fact  is 
that  the  mechanism  of  action  potential  production  is 
verv  different  in  difTerent  tissues. 


The  author  wishes  to  express  his  gratitude  to  the  following 
colleagues  who  have  kindly  read  the  manuscript  of  this  chapter 
and  have  given  many  important  suggestions;  Dr.  M.  Fuortes, 
Dr.  S.  Hagiwara,  Prof.  A.  L.  Hodgkin  and  Dr.  C.  S.  Spyro- 
poulos.  The  manuscript  was  prepared  with  the  valuable  help 
of  Mrs.  Mary  Allen,  Mrs.  Claire  Mayer  and  Mrs.  Lydia  N. 
Tasaki,  to  whom  the  author  also  wants  to  express  his  apprecia- 
tion. 


REFERENCES 


1.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.  45:  389,   191 2. 

2.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.  55:  193,  1921. 

3.  Adrian,  E.  D.  The  Basis  oj  Sensation.  London:  Christo- 
phers, 1928. 

4.  Adrian,  E.  D.  The  .Mechanism  oJ  Nervous  Ac/ion.  Phila- 
delphia: Univ.  Pennsylvania  Press,  1932. 

5.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  D.  W.  Bronk.  J.  Physiol.  66:  81,  1928. 

6.  Adrian,   E.  D.   and  K.  Lucas.  J.  Physiol.  44:  68,   1912. 

7.  Adrian,  R.  H.  J.  Physiol.    133:  631,    1956. 

8.  Arvanitaki,  a.  Proprtetes  Rytlimiqurs  de  la  .Maliere  17- 
vante.  Variations  graduees  de  la  fmlarisaluni  el  rylhimcites. 
Paris:  Hermann,  1938. 

9.  Arvanitaki,  A.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  8g,  1942. 

10.  Bernstein,  J.  Elektrohiologie.  Braunschweig:  Vieweg, 
1912. 

11.  Beutner,   R.   Biochem.  ^tschr.    137:  496,    1923. 

12.  Biedermann,  W.  Elektrophysiologie.  Jena:  Fischer,  1895. 
(English  translation  by  F.  A.  VVelby.  London:  Mac- 
millan,  1896,  1898.) 

13.  Bishop,  G.  H.,  P.  Heinbecker  and  J.  L.  O'Learv.  Am. 
J.  Physiol.  106:  647,  1933. 

14.  Blair,  E.  A.  and  J.  Erlanger.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  114: 
309.  1935/36. 


15.  Blair,  H.  .\.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  15:  709,  1932. 

16.  BowDiTCH,  H.  P.  1871.  (Cited  in  J.  F.  Fulton.  AIuscu- 
lar  Contraction.  Baltimore:  Williams  &  Wilkins,  1926,  p. 
48.) 

17.  BovLE,  P.  J.  AND  E.  J.  CoNWAV.  J.  Physiol.  100:  I,  1941. 

18.  Cole,  K.  .S.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  25:  29,  1941. 

19.  Cole,  K.  S.  and  H.  J.  Curtis.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  649, 

1939- 

20.  Cole,  K.  S.  and  A.  L.  Hodgkin.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  671, 

'939- 

21.  Craib,  W.  H.  J.  Physiol.  66:  49,  1928. 

22.  Curtis,  H.  J.  and  K.  S.  Cole.  J.  Cell.  &  Cornp.  Physiol. 

15:  147.  1940- 

23.  Davis,  H.,  A.  Forbes,  D.  Brunswick  and  A.  McH. 
Hopkins.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  76:  448,   1926. 

24.  Downing,  A.  C,  R.  W.  Gerard  and  A.  V.  Hill.  Proc. 
Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B   100:  223,    1926. 

25.  DU  Bois-Revmond,  E.  Unlersuehungen  iiber  Thierische  Elek- 
Iricitdt.  Berlin:  G.   Reimer,    1848/49. 

26.  Eccles,  J.  C.  and  C.  S.  Sherrington.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc, 
London,  ser.  5  106:  326,  1930. 

27.  Erlanger,  J.  and  E.  A.  Blair.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.  99:  129, 
1931/32- 


HANDBOOK    OF    FHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


28. 
29- 

30- 
3'- 

33- 

34- 

35- 

36. 

37- 
38. 

39- 

40. 

41- 
4-2. 

43- 

44- 

45- 
46. 

47- 
48. 

49- 
50- 
5'- 

5^- 
53- 
54- 
55- 
56. 

57- 

58. 

59- 

60. 

61. 

62. 
63- 

64. 


Erlanger,  J.    AND    E.    A.    Blair.    Am.    J.    Physiol,    iio: 

287,  1934. 

Erlanger,  J.    and    E.    A.    Blair.    .4m.   J.    Fhstol.    114: 

328,  1935/36- 

Fatt,  p.  and  B.  Katz.   7.  Phmol.   115:  3J0,  1951. 

Fatt,  p.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  120;  171,  1953. 

Fenn,  W.  O.  J.  Gen.  Phy.uol.  10:  767,  1927. 

Fessard,   a.    Proprietes   Rythmiques   de  la    Maliere    Vivante. 

Paris:  Hermann,   1936,  Part  I  (p.  99),   Part  II  (p.  66). 

Forbes,   .\.   and   C.   Thacher.   Am.  J.   Physiol.   52 :  409, 

1920. 

Franck,   U.   F.   Pro^.   Biophys.  &   Biophys.   Chem.   6:    171, 

1956- 

Frankenhauser,  B.  J.  Physiol.   118:   107,   1952. 

Frankenhauser,  B.   and  D.  Schneider.  J.  Physiol.    115: 

i'7.  I95"- 

Gasser,  H.  S.  and  J.  Erlanger.  Electrical  Signs  oj  yerivu! 
Action.  Philadelphia:  Univ.  Pennsylvania  Press,  1937. 
Cesser,  H.  S.  .\nd  H.  T.  Graham,  .im.  J.  Physiol.  loi: 
316,  1932. 

Gerard,  R.  W.  Physiol.  Rei\    12:  469,    1932. 
Gilson,  a.  S.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  1 16:  358,  1936. 
Goldman,  D.  E.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  27:  37,   1943. 
Grundfest,  H.   In:  .Modern   Trends  in  Physiology  and  Bio- 
chemistry, edited  by   E.   S.   G.   Barron.   New  York:  .Acad. 
Press,  1952. 

Grundfest,  H.  In:  Electrochemistry  in  Biology  and  .Medi- 
cine, edited  by  T.  Shedlovsky.  New  York:  Wiley,  1955, 
p.  141. 

Grundfest,  H.,  A.   M.  Sh.-^nes  and  W.   Frevgang.  J. 
Gen.  Physiol.  37:  25,  1953. 
Hagiwara,  S.  and  .a.  \V.\tanabe.  J.  Physiol.    129:  513, 

1955- 

Hermann,  L.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  75:  574,  1899. 

Hill   A.  V.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  119:  305,  1936. 

Hober,  R.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  106:  599,  1905. 

Hodgkin,  a.  L.  J.  Physiol.  90:  183,  1937. 

HoDGKiN,  A.   L.   Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.   B   126:  87, 

,938. 

Hodgkin,  .A.  L.  J.  Ph\siol.  94:  560,  1939. 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  J.  Physiol.  106:  319,   1947. 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  J.  Physiol.   107:   165,   1948. 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  Biol.  Rev.  26:  339,  1951. 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  A.  F.  Huxley.  Nature,  London   144: 

7'0,  1939- 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  .A.  F.  Huxley.  J.  Physiol.  116:  449, 


1952- 
Hodgkin, 

473.  1952- 

Hodgkin,   .\.    L.    and   .\ 

500,  1952. 

Hodgkin,   A 


.A.    L.    AND    .\.    F.    Huxley.   J.    Physiol.    116: 

Huxley.   J.    Physiol.    117: 

.\.    F.    Huxley.   J.    Physiol.    121: 


403.  ■953- 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.,  A.  F.  Huxley,  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Phys- 
iol. 116:  424,  1952. 

Hodgkin,  A.  L.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.   108:  37,   1949. 
Hodgkin,    A.    L.    and    \V.    A.    H.    Rushton.    Proc.    Roy. 
Soc,  London,  ser.  B  133:  444,  1946. 
Hodler,  J.,  R.  St.ampfli  and  I.  Tasaki.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

170;  375.  1952- 

Hodler,  J.,  R.  Stampfli  and  I.  Tasaki.  Helvet.  physiol.  et 

Pharmacol,  acta  10:  C54,  1952. 


66.  Hl'xley 

67, 
68 
69 


1949- 
Huxley, 

I95"- 
Huxley, 

'95'- 


A.   F.   AND   R.   Stampfli.  J.  Physiol.    108:  315, 
.\.  F.  AND  R.  Stampfli.  J.  Physiol.   112:  476, 

.•\.    F.    AND    R.    .ST.«iMPFLI.    J.    Physinl.     112:    496, 


Kato,  G.  The  Theory  of  Decrementless  Conduction  in  Narcotized 
Region  of  .Verve.  Tokyo ;  Nankodo,  1 924. 

70.  Kato,  G.    The  Microphysiology  of  Nerve.  Tokyo:  Maruzen, 

>934- 

71.  K.\TO,   G.    Cold  Spring   Harbor  Symp.   Qiiant.   Biol.   4:   202, 

1936- 

72.  K.\TZ,    B.    Electric   Excitation   of  .Nerve.    London:   Oxford, 

1939- 

73.  K.\TZ,  B.  .-^ND  O.  H.  ScHMiTT.  f.  Physiol.  97:  471,  1940. 

74.  Keynes,  R.  D.  J.  Physiol.  114:  119,  1951. 

75.  LiLLIE,  R.  S.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  7:  473,  1925. 

76.  Ling,  G.  and  R.  \V.  Gerard.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  34: 

383.  1949- 

77.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  .4  .Study  of  Nerve  Physiology  (Parts  I 
and  II).  New  York:  Rockefeller  Inst.  Med.  Res.  1947. 

78.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  33 :  Suppl.  i , 

'949- 

79.  Lucks,  K.  J.  Physiol.  36:  253,  1907. 

80.  Luc.'KS,  K.  J.  Physiol.  37:  112,  1908. 

81.  Lucas,  K.  J.  Physiol.  38:  113,  1909. 

82.  Luc.\s,  K.  The  Conduction  of  the  Nervous  Impulse.  London: 
Longmans,  1917. 

83.  Marmont,  G.  .-Ini.  J.  Physiol.   130:  392,  1940. 

84.  Marmont,  G.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  34:  351,  1949. 

85.  Marrazzi,  .^.  S.  AND  R.  LoRENTE  DE  N6.  J.  Neurophysiot. 
7:  83,  1944. 

86.  M.ilTteucci,  Carlo.  Compt.  rend.  Acad.  Sc  Pans  14:  310, 
1842. 

87.  McNiCHOL,  E.  F.  AND  H.  Wagner.  Naval  Med.  Res.  Inst. 
Project  Report,  Vol.  12,  p.  97-1 18,  1954. 

88.  Monnier,  a.  M.  L' Excitation  Electrique  des  Tissues.  Pai-is: 
Hermann,  1934. 

89.  Nachm.\noshn,  D.  The  Harvey  Lectures.  Series  XLIX.  New 
York:  .Acad.  Press,  1955. 

qo.  Nachmansohn,  D.  and  I.  B.  Wilson.  In:  Electrochemistry 
in  Biology  and  .Medicine,  edited  by  T.  Shedio\sky.  New 
York:  Wiley,  1955,  p-  167. 

91.  Nernst,  W.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  122:  275,  1908. 

92.  Niedergerke,  R.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  258:  108,  1953. 

93.  Osterhout,  W.  J.  V.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  7,  1941. 

94.  Osterhout,  W.  J.  V.  In:  Electrochemistry  in  Biology  and 
.Medicine,   edited   by   T.    .Shedlossky.    New   York :   Wiley, 

1955.  P-  213- 

95.  Osterhout,  W.J.  V.  and  S.  E.  Hill.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.   13: 

547.  1930- 

96.  Otani,  T.  Jap.  J.  .Med.  Sc.  Ill  Biophys.  4:  355,  1937. 

97.  Overton,  E.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  92;  346,  1902. 

98.  Parker,  G.  H.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  8:  21,  1925. 

99.  Pecher,  C.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  122:  87,  1936. 

100.   Pflijger,  E.  F.  W.   Untersuchungen  iiber  die  Physiologic  des 

Elektrotonus.  Berlin,  1859. 
loi.   Pr.\tt,   F.   H.    and  J.    P.   Eisenburger.   Am.  J.   Physiol. 

49:  I,  1919. 
102.   Rashevsky,   N.    .Mathematical    Biophysics.    Chicago:   Univ. 

Chicago  Press,  1938. 


CONDUCTION    OF    THE    NERVE    IMPULSE 


103. 
104. 
105. 
106. 
107. 
108. 
109. 


113- 
114. 

i'5- 
116. 

117. 
118. 
119. 
120. 
121. 

I -22. 
123. 

124. 


RosENBLUETH,  A.,  J.  A.  Alanis  AND  J.  Mandoki.  J.  Cell.  126. 

&  Comp.  Physiol.  33:  405,  1949-  127. 

RosENBLUETH,    A.    AND    J.    V.    Luco.   J.    Cell.  &    Comp. 
Physiol.  36:  289,  1950. 

RusHTON,  VV.   A.   H.   Pioc.   Roy.   .Soc,   London,    ier.   B    124:  128. 

210,  1937.  129. 

ScHMiTT,  O.  H.  In:  Electrochemistry  in  Biolooy  and  .Medicine^ 
edited  by  T.  Shedlovsky.  New  York:  Wiley,  1955,  p-  91-  130. 

SCHOEFFLE,   G.   M.    AND  J.    ErLANGER.   Am.  J.    Plnsiol.    I  67  : 

134.  1951-  '3i- 

Shanes,  A.  M.,  H.  Grundfest  and  \V.  Frevgang.  J. 
Gen.  Physiol.  37 :  39,  1 953.  1 32. 

Shaw,  F.  H.,  S.  E.  Simon,  B.  M.  Johnstone  and  M.  E. 
Holman.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  40:  263,  1956.  133. 

Shedlovsky,  T.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Qiianl.  Biol.   17: 

97.  195^-  '34- 

Shedlovsky,    T.    (editor).    Electrochemistry   in    Biology   and 

Medicine.  New  York:  Wiley,  1955.  135- 

Sollner,   K.    In :   Electrochemistry  in   Biology  and  .Medicine, 

edited  by  T.  Shedlovsky.  New  York:  Wiley,  1955,  p.  33.  136. 

Stampfli,  R.  Ergebn.  Physiol.  47;  70,  1952. 

Stampfli,  R.  Physiol.  Rev.  34:  loi,  1954.  137. 

St.ampfli,    R.    and    Y.    Zotterman.    Helvet.    physiol.    et 

Pharmacol,  acta  g.  208,  igji.  138. 

Takeuchi,  T.  and  I.  Tasakl  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  246:  32,  139. 

1942.  140. 

Tasaki,  I.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  127:  211,  1939.  141. 

Tasaki,  I.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  245:  665,  1942. 

Tasaki,  I.  Biochim.  el  biophys.  acta  3;  498,  1949.  142. 

Tasaki,  I.  Biochim.  et  biophys.  acta  3:  665,  1949.  143. 

Tasaki,  I.  Cytologia  15:  205,  1950. 

Tasaki,  I.  Jap.  J.  Physiol,  i :  75,  1950.  144. 

Tasaki,  I.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Qitanl.  Biol.    17:  37,  145. 

■952- 

Tasaki,  I.  .Nervous  Transmission.  Springfield :  Thomas,  1953.  146. 

Tasaki,  I.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  181 :  639,  1955. 


Tasaki,  I.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  377,  1956. 
Tas.iki,     I.     In:    Microphysiologie    Comparee    des    Elements 
Excitables.  Colloques  Internationaux  du  Centre  National 
de  la  Recherche  Scientilique  No.  67,  Paris,  1957,  p.  i. 
Tasaki,  I.  and  K.  Fr.ank.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  182:  572,  1955. 
T.AS.AKi,  I.  .AND  W.  H.  Freyc.^ng,  Jr.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

211,  1955- 

T.AS.AKi,   I.  AND  S.   H.Acnv.AR.A.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.    188:  423, 

■957- 

Tas.aki,   I.,   K..    IsHii   AND   H.    Ito.  Jap.  J.   .Med.  Sc.   HI 

Biophys.  9:  189,  1943. 

T.AS.AKI,  I.  AND  K.  MizuGUCHi.  J.  .Veurophytiol.   1 1  :  295, 

1948. 

Tas.aki,  I.  AND  K.  MizuGucHi.  Biochern  et  biophys.  acta  3: 

484,  1949. 

Tasaki,  I.  and  K.  Mizutani.  Jap.  J.  .Med.  Sc.  HI  Biophys. 

10:  245.  '944- 

Tasaki,  I.  .and  T.  Takeuchi.  .irch.  ges.  Physiol.  244:  696, 

'941- 

Tasaki,  I.  and  T.  Takeuchi.  .-irch.  gei.  Physiol.  245:  764, 

1942- 

Tasaki,  I.  and  N.  Tas.aki.  Biophys.  et  biochim.  acta  5:  335, 

'950- 

T.\SHiRO,  S.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  32:  107,  1913. 

Teorell,  T.  Prog.  Biophys.  &  Biophys.  Chem.  3:  305,  1953. 

Verworn,  M.  Irritability.  New  Haven:Yale,  1913. 

VON    Helmholtz,    H.    Arch.     Anal.     Physiol.     71,     1850. 

(Cited  in  Biedermann,  12.) 

Weidmann,  S.  J.  Physiol.  1 15:  227,  1951. 

Weidmann,   S.   Elektrophysiologie  der  Herzmuskeljaser.   Bern 

und  Stuttgart:  Huber,  1956. 

Winterstein,  H.  Die  .Narkose.  Berlin:J.  Springer,  1926. 

WoLFGRAM,  F.  J.  AND  A.  VAN  Harreveld.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

171:  140,  1952- 

Young,  J.   Z.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.   (hianl.   Biol.  4:   I, 
1936. 


CHAPTER    IV 


Initiation  of  impulses  at  receptors 


J.  A.  B.  GRAY      I      Department  oj  Physiology,  University  College,  London,  England 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

General  Properties 

Type  of  Energy  Required  to  Excite  Receptors 

Adaptation 

Receptive  Fields 

Information 
Repetitive  Responses  and  Tonic  Receptors 

Stimulus-Frequency  Relations 

The  Effect  of  a  Reduction  of  Excitation 

The  Nature  of  Repetitive  Firing 

Modification  of  Afferent  Discharges  by  Current 
Excitation  of  Impulses  by  Controlled   Pulses  and  Phasic  Re- 
ceptors 

Quantitative  Aspects  of  Excitation 

On  and  Off  Responses 

Summation 
Receptor  Potentials  and  Other  Generator  Potentials 

Generator  Potentials  in  Complex  Organs 

Receptor  Potentials  Generated  in  Nerve  Terminals 

Relation  of  Receptor  Potentials  to  Impulse  Initiation 

Quantitative    Relations    between    Stimulus    and    Receptor 
Potential 

Absolute  Magnitude  of  the  Receptor  Potential 

Summation  of  Receptor  Potentials 

Depression 
Site  of  Impulse  Initiation 

Effect  of  Procaine  and  Sodium  Lack  on  Receptor  Potentials 
Transmission  of  Energy  to  the  Receptor  Elements 
Effects  of  'Transmitter'  Substances 

Action  of  Acetylcholine 

Action  of  Blocking  Agents  and  Anticholinesterases 

Effects  of  Sympathetic  Stimulation  and  Epinephrine 

Other  Substances 
Minute  Structure  of  Receptors 
Hypotheses  Concerning  the  Mechanisms  of  Receptors 


IN  THE  INTACT  ORGANISM  impulses  are  set  up  in  pri- 
mary afferent  fibers  as  a  result  of  activity  in  those 
receptors  with  which  the  fibers  are  associated.  These 
receptors  may  consist  solely  of  specialized  termina- 
tions of  the  afferent  nerve  fibers,  or  the  nerve  endings 


may  be  associated  with  other  cells  which  play  a 
significant  role  in  the  initiation  of  impulses.  In  either 
instance,  the  role  of  the  receptor  is  to  record  the 
state  of,  or  changes  in,  the  physical  or  chemical  en- 
vironment by  the  initiation  of  impulses  which  are 
then  conducted  in  the  primary  afferent  fibers  to  the 
central  nervous  system.  A  primary  afferent  fiber  may 
be  connected  with  a  single  receptor  or  with  many; 
but  even  when  it  is  supplied  by  numerous  receptors, 
a  single  afferent  fiber  remains  a  single  channel  into 
the  central  nervous  system  and  must  be  considered  as 
such.  When  dealing  with  the  activity  in  such  a  fiber 
it  is  necessary  to  consider  the  fiber  and  all  its  periph- 
eral connections  as  a  whole,  that  is  as  a  sen.sory  unit. 
It  is  the  purpose  of  this  section  to  consider  something 
of  the  general  behas'ior  of  sensory  units  and  of  the 
mechanisms  by  which  indi\idual  receptors  initiate 
impulses  in  the  primary  afferent  neurons. 


GENERAL    PROPERTIES 

A  few  words  should  first  be  said  concerning  classi- 
fication. Sensory  units  can  be  described  by  reference 
to  the  properties  of  the  specific  stimulus,  the  nature 
of  the  activity  and  the  site  and  distribution  of  the  re- 
ceptive field.  All  these  factors,  together  with  the  con- 
duction velocity  of  the  fiber,  are  measurable  quanti- 
ties and  a  precise  description  of  a  sensory  unit  is  thus 
possible.  It  seems  better  in  this  context  to  avoid  terms 
such  as  warmth,  pain  or  red;  these  terms  describe 
sensations  which  depend  on  the  activity  of  the  whole 
nervous  system,  not  just  on  the  properties  of  one 
sensory  unit. 

Type  of  Energy  Required  to  Excite  Receptors 

In  most  animal  organisms  there  are  receptors  that 
respond  to  the  following  forms  of  energy:  mechanical, 


123 


[24 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


thermal,  electromagnetic  (as  light)  and  chemical. 
With  the  exception  of  one  important  group,  it  ap- 
pears that  nearly  all  receptors  are  especially  sensitive 
to  one  form  of  energy  and  either  completely  or  rela- 
tively insensitive  to  all  others.  The  excepted  group 
consists  of  those  receptors  that  have  a  low  sensitivity 
to  all  types  of  energy,  but  will  respond  to  any  form 
of  energy  that  reaches  a  damaging  or  near  damaging 
level;  these  receptors  are  of  course  those  that  give 
rise  to  defensive  reflexes  and  are  associated  with  the 
sensation  of  pain.  The  specificity  of  receptors  to  a 
particular  form  of  energy  was  first  propounded  in 
modern  times  by  Miiller  (76).  As  a  whole  this  con- 
cept is  not  seriously  challenged,  but  recently  an 
attack  has  been  made  on  its  application  to  receptors 
situated  in  the  skin  (99).  The  objections  are  based 
both  on  the  finding  that  there  are  areas  of  human 
skin  in  which  morphologically  specialized  endings 
are  not  found  but  from  which  all  modalities  of  sen- 
sation can  be  elicited  (39),  and  on  the  results  of 
certain  sensation  experiments  (65,  66).  There  need 
be  no  rigid  correlation  between  morphological  and 
functional  specializations;  it  is  indeed  interesting  to 
note  that  the  bulk  of  the  direct  evidence  for  the  func- 
tional specificity  of  sensory  units  has  come  from 
preparations  of  frog  and  toad  skin  which  contain 
few  morphologically  differentiated  nerve  endings 
(24).  As  regards  to  sen.sation  experiments  it  must  be 
realized  that  sensations  are  the  end  result  of  compli- 
cated processes  and  that  such  experiments,  while 
giving  information  about  sensations  and  their  specific- 
ity, cannot  weigh  heavily  against  direct  evidence  on 
the  properties  of  sensory  units. 

Direct  evidence  of  the  specificity  of  units  has  been 
obtained  by  recording  the  responses  of  single  ones  to 
different  stimuli.  In  the  earlier  experiments  of  this 
type  single  fibers  were  not  isolated  in  an  anatomical 
sense,  but  small  bundles  of  nerve  fibers  were  used  so 
that  the  activity  of  indi\idual  units  could  he  identi- 
fied and  analyzed;  using  this  technique  it  was  possible 
to  show  that  therrnal  and  near-damaging  stimuli 
only  excited  activity  in  small  fibers  and  did  not 
produce  activity  in  the  larger  fibers  which  responded 
only  to  mechanical  stimuli  (2,  50,  103,  104).  This 
type  of  work  has  now  been  carried  a  stage  further  by 
isolating  and  recording  from  single  afferent  fibers 
that  have  their  receptive  fields  in  the  skin  of  toads  and 
of  cats  (73).  Results  of  such  experiments  show  that 
mechanically  sensitive  units  are  not  easily  excited  by 
thermal  stimuli  [though  this  has  been  shown  to  hap- 
pen in  the  cat's  tongue  (45)]  or  by  acid;  that  ther- 
mally  sensitive   units   do   not    normally    respond    to 


mechanical  stimulation  [the  rattlesnake  pit  organ  is 
an  exception  (15)];  and  that  units  responding  to 
acid,  prick  or  burning  do  not  respond  to  small 
mechanical  or  thermal  stimuli. 

The  specificity  of  sensory  units  is  not  confined, 
however,  to  a  simple  distinction  between  different 
types  of  energy  but  involves  distinctions  between 
other  properties  of  the  stimulus.  Of  these  properties, 
those  connected  with  its  time  course  are  perhaps  the 
most  obvious;  the  different  rates  of  adaptation  ex- 
hibited by  different  units  is  an  example  which  will 
be  considered  again.  .Specificity  to  a  particular  band 
of  frequencies  of  a  periodic  function  is  another  ex- 
ample; thus  there  is  evidence  that  different  units  in 
the  retina  respond  to  different  frequencies  of  light 
waves  (31,  91)  and  that  primary  units  from  the 
mammalian  cochlea  have  particular  characteristics 
in  relation  to  the  frequency  of  the  sound  waves  (93). 
In  both  these  instances  the  sensory  units  are  display- 
ing a  specificity,  ijut  there  is  clearly  a  difference  in 
the  way  this  specificity  is  brought  about.  In  the 
retina  it  seems  probable  that  individual  receptors  are 
different,  but  in  the  cochlea  it  is  the  mechanical 
properties  of  the  system  that  are  mainly  responsible 
for  the  results.  Such  a  distinction  between  the  proper- 
ties of  the  receptor  and  the  properties  of  the  support- 
ing tissues  is  one  that  ari.ses  in  other  situations  but  is 
one  that  is  irrelevant  in  the  context  of  describing  the 
properties  of  sensory  units.  The  examples  of  specificity 
so  far  given  in  this  paragraph  are  concerned  with 
time  factors,  but  there  are  others.  Thus  there  are 
two  types  of  thermal  unit  found  in  the  cat's  tongue; 
in  both  types  the  frequency  of  the  impulse  discharge 
depends  on  the  temperature  of  the  receptors,  but  in 
one  group  the  maximum  frequency  is  found  at  a 
temperature  of  30  to  32 °C  (46),  while  in  the  other  it 
occurs  at  37.5  to  4o°C  (22).  Again,  units  in  the  cat's 
tongue  responding  to  chemical  stimuli,  and  pre- 
sumably responsible  for  the  sensation  of  taste,  can 
be  grouped  in  respect  to  the  substances  that  are  able 
to  set  up  activity  in  them  (82). 

Adaptation 

When  a  piece  of  tissue  containing  a  receptor  sensi- 
tive to  mechanical  stimuli  is  subjected  to  an  abrupt 
increase  in  the  forces  applied  to  it  and  the  new  situa- 
tion is  then  maintained,  the  sensory  unit  will  dis- 
charge impulses  at  a  frequency,  which  starts  at  a 
relatively  high  value  and  then  decreases  with  time 
(4,  74,  75)  (fig.  i).  This  decline  in  frequency  is  known 
as  adaptation  and  may  be  slow  or  rapid.   In  those 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


12- 


units  that  are  described  as  tonic,  the  frequency  of 
the  impulse  discharge  declines  relatively  slowly  to  a 
steady  value  which  is  characteristic  of  the  applied 
force  (fig.  i);  the  frequency  of  the  discharge  from 
other  units,  those  called  phasic,  adapts  more  rapidly 
and  finally  falls  to  zero  (5).  In  an  extreme  case  a 
sensory  unit  may  only  discharge  a  single  impulse 
during  the  change  in  the  applied  forces  and  will 
then  remain  silent  until  another  change  takes  place. 
Adaptation  is  also  observed  in  sensory  units  specifi- 
cally sensitive  to  forms  of  energy  other  than  mechani- 
cal. 

Adaptation  is  a  word  that  describes  the  response  of 
a  sensory  unit  to  a  particular  function  of  the  type  of 
energy  concerned.  When  adaptation  is  rapid,  it  can 
be  said  that  the  unit  is  not  signalling  a  pressure,  a 
temperature  or  a  concentration;  but  it  does  not  tell 
us  what  particular  function  in  respect  to  time  is 
signalled. 

To  say  that  a  function  is  signalled  means  that  a 
constant  value  of  the  function  gives  rise  to  a  constant 
and  repeatable  frequency  of  impulse  discharge  in  the 
fiber  of  the  sensory  unit.  In  most  situations  it  is  very 
difficult  to  maintain  a  constant  velocity  or  accelera- 
tion for  a  sufficiently  long  time  to  see  whether  or  not 
a  constant  frequency  of  discharge  is  in  fact  set  up 
[a  notable  exception  has  appeared  in  the  experiments 
on  the  semicircular  canals  using  constant  angular 
velocities  and  accelerations  (72)].  Even  if  such  ex- 
periments were  performed  it  is  by  no  means  certain 
that  simple  relations  would  be  found.  This  is  there- 


fore a  situation  in  which  it  is  necessary  to  continue  to 
use  an  empirical  description. 

It  should  be  noted  that  in  the  first  sentence  of  this 
section,  reference  is  made  to  the  tissue  surrounding 
the  receptors.  Even  in  the  instances  in  which  a  recep- 
tor has  been  isolated,  e.g.  the  muscle  spindle  and 
Pacinian  corpuscle,  there  is  far  more  supporting  tis- 
sue than  active  element.  These  supporting  tissues 
may  be  of  fundamental  importance  in  the  adapta- 
tion of  'simple'  receptors  in  the  same  way  as  the 
structures  of  the  middle  ear  and  cochlea  cause 
'adaptation'  of  the  ear  to  steady  pressures  applied 
to  the  tympanic  membrane.  This  problem  will  be 
considered  at  a  later  stage  when  all  the  relevant 
evidence   has   been  discussed. 

Receptive  Fields 

A  sensory  unit  has  a  particular  situation  and  par- 
ticular size  of  receptive  field,  i.e.  the  area  from  which 
the  single  afferent  fiber  receives  branches.  The  size 
of  these  receptive  fields  can  vary  quite  considerably, 
for  example  up  to  9  by  5  cm,  not  mm,  in  cat's  skin 
(73)  and  up  to  100  sq.  mm  in  frog's  skin  (3);  while 
other  sensory  units  have  receptive  fields  which  com- 
prise only  a  single  end  organ.  Variation  of  size  of  re- 
ceptive fields  occurs  with  different  types  of  unit  in 
skin  and  also  in  specialized  organs  such  as  the  eye. 
There  is  wide  overlap  of  receptive  fields  and  it  is 
clear  that  spatial  discrimination  must  depend  on  the 
coordination  of  information  supplied  through  a  con- 
sideraljle  number  of  primary  channels. 


FIG.  I.  Response  of  cat  muscle  spindle  to  stretch.  Abscissa: 
time  in  sec.  Ordinate:  impulses  frequency  per  sec.  Each  curve 
for  a  diflferent  force.  [From  Matthews  (75).] 


Information 

Sensory  units  constitute  independent  channels 
which  signal  to  the  central  nervous  system  informa- 
tion about  the  physical  and  chemical  environment  of 
the  organism.  This  information  is  conveyed  by  the 
pattern  of  activity  in  any  one  unit  and  by  the  charac- 
teristics and  organization  of  each  channel.  These 
factors  can  be  classified  as  follows: 
a)  Factors  related  to  time 

i)     Interval  between  impulses 
ii)    Duration  of  activity 
h)  Factors  related  to  the  properties  of  units 

i)     Characteristics    of    the    'normal'    stimulus, 
e.g.    the   nature   of   the   energy   and   other 
relevant  factors 
ii)    Size  and  position  of  the  receptive  field 
iii)  SensitivitN'  of  the  unit 


i->6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


REPETITIVE  RESPONSES  AND  TONIG  RECEPTORS 

Stimulus-Frequency  Relations 

There  are  many  sensory  units,  the  function  of 
which  is  to  signal  to  the  central  nervous  system  the 
properties  of  a  steady  state,  e.g.  temperature,  con- 
centration or  intensity  of  illumination.  At  any  time, 
except  shortly  after  an  aljrupt  change  from  one  state 
to  another,  the  frequency  of  the  impulse  discharge 
of  the  unit  will  depend  on  the  value  of  the  physical 
or  chemical  function  in  question;  and  a  particular 
frequency  will,  in  the  working  range  of  any  one  unit, 
be  consistently  related  to  a  particular  value.  One 
example  is  seen  in  figure  2  where  the  frequency  of 
impulse  discharge  in  five  single  fibers  from  pressure 
receptors  of  the  carotid  sinus  is  plotted  against  pres- 
sure in  the  sinus.  Another  example  appears  in  figm-e 
22  of  Chapter  XVIII  on  Thermal  Sensations  in  this 
volume  (p.  452),  in  which  the  impulse  frequencies  in 
two  units  from  the  cat's  tongue  responding  to  thermal 
stimuli  are  plotted  against  temperature.  The  curves 
in  the  two  figures  are  clearly  quite  different;  the 
pressure  units,  while  showing  individual  variations, 
all  start  to  fire  at  a  certain  pressure  above  which  the 
frequency  of  discharge  increa.ses  as  the  pressure  in- 
creases until  an  upper  limit  of  frequency  is  reached 
(12,  60).  The  temperature  units  on  the  other  hand 
both  show  maxima  in  their  temperature-frequency 
relationship,  but  these  maxima  occur  at  two  widely 
different  temperatures.  The  two  types  of  response 
represent  the  activity  of  two  distinct  groups  of  units 
found  in  cats  (22,  46)  and  it  is  presumed  that  the 


FIG.  2.  Responses  of  five  single  pressure  sensitive  units  QA 
to  £)  from  the  cat's  carotid  sinus.  Abscissa:  intrasinusal  pres- 
sure in  mm  Hg.  Ordinate:  impulse  frequency  per  sec.  [From 
Landgren  (60).] 


activity  of  these  two  types  of  unit  bears  a  close  causal 
relationship  with  the  subjective  sensations  of  cold 
and  warmth. 

Looking  at  the  two  examples  shown,  it  would  seem 
improbable  that  any  relationship  between  'stimulus' 
and  frequency  having  general  relevance  to  sensory 
units  of  all  types  could  be  found.  This  is  strictly  true, 
but  there  is  a  relationship  that  has  been  found  to  de- 
scribe reasonably  well  the  response  characteristics  of 
certain  types  of  unit  in  their  working  range.  This  is 
what  is  known  as  the  Weber-Fechner  law.  This  law 
derives  from  an  observation  made  by  Weber  that  the 
smallest  difference  in  the  weight  of  two  objects  bears 
a  constant  relation  to  the  weight  of  the  objects.  It  is 
usually  given  as  AI/I  =  C,  where  /  is  intensity  of 
stimulus.  A/  the  smallest  detectable  difference  in 
intensity  and  C  a  constant.  Fechner  developed  this 
observation  in  a  theoretical  way  by  making  the  as- 
sumption that  each  discriminable  step  of  stimulus 
intensity  corresponds  to  a  imit  increase  in  sensation, 
that  is  to  say  he  stated  that  AI/I  =  kAS  where  AS 
is  the  increase  in  sensation.  From  this  it  follows  that 
d5/d/  =  I /k/  and  S  =  a  log  /  +  A.  This  equation 
was  originally  put  forward  in  an  attempt  to  quanti- 
tate  .sensation,  a  thing  we  are  not  concerned  with 
here;  howe\er,  we  are  concerned  with  its  relevance 
to  'stimulus' -frequency  relations.  The  relation  be- 
tween the  applied  force  and  impulse  frequency  re- 
corded from  a  frog's  muscle  spindle  has  been  found 
to  be  consistent  with  this  relationship  (95).  The  cor- 
responding relationship  between  intensity  of  illumina- 
tion and  response  from  an  ommatidium  of  the  eye  of 
Limiilus  is  also  consistent  with  the  equation  under 
certain  specific  conditions  (41).  These  findings  have 
inevitably  raised  the  question  of  whether  this  rela- 
tionship indicates  anything  about  the  mechanisms 
invoked  in  the  initiation  of  impulses  or  whether  it 
must  be  regarded  simply  as  an  empirical  description 
(31).  The  fit  between  equation  and  experiment  is  not 
sufficiently  good  to  suggest  that  the  fundamental 
processes  depend  on  a  simple  logarithmic  relation- 
ship, but  if,  as  seems  possible,  the.se  processes  are 
related  to  ionic  equilibria  across  cell  membranes,  a 
logarithmic  term  might  he  expected  to  appear  in  the 
relationship. 

Effect  fij  a  Reduction  oj  Excitation 

It  has  already  been  pointed  out  that  while  a  tonic 
sensory  unit  will  respond  to  a  certain  steady  state 
with  a  certain  frequency,  a  sudden  increase  in,  for 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


127 


instance,  the  applied  force  will  cause  a  relatively  large 
increase  in  the  frequency  of  the  discharge,  an  increase 
which  will  then  decline  until  the  correct  frequency 
for  the  new  steady  state  has  been  reached.  A  similar 
process  occurs  if  there  is  a  sudden  decrease  in,  again 
for  instance,  the  applied  force.  In  this  instance  the 
frequency  falls  abruptly  to  a  value  below  that  ex- 
pected for  the  new  steady  state  and  then  increases 
with  time.  Thus,  if  a  muscle  spindle  is  discharging 
rhythmically  and  the  muscle  in  which  it  lies  is 
stretched  for  a  time  and  then  suddenly  returned  to 
its  resting  length,  the  frequency  of  the  discharge  from 
the  spindle  falls  well  below  its  resting  value,  possibly 
to  zero;  after  a  time  the  resting  rhythm  re-establishes 
itself  (75).  Similar  changes  can  be  observed  in  other 
types  of  unit,  for  example  in  temperature  sensitive 
units  (46),  and  the  pressure  sensiti\e  units  of  the  cat's 
carotid  sinus  (60).  It  should  be  noted  that  the  beha- 
vior of  such  units  contrasts  with  that  of  phasic  units 
which  are  considered  in  another  section  below. 

Nature  of  Repetitive  Firing 

Ideas  on  the  mechanisms  by  which  firing  takes 
place  started  with  the  proposals  of  Adrian  (i).  Essen- 
tially these  were  that  special  nonaccommodating 
regions  of  nerve  exist  at  sensory  nerve  endings  and 
that  repetitive  activity  is  initiated  in  these  regions;  the 
frequency  of  the  discharge  depends  on  the  refractory 
period  which  may  be  longer  here  than  in  other  parts 
of  the  nerve.  Broadly  speaking,  work  on  the  nature  of 
repetitive  firing  by  sensory  receptors  has  followed  two 
lines.  The  first  has  attacked  the  problem  of  nerve 
accommodation  and  the  other,  the  mechanism  that 
determines  the  interval  between  impulses. 

Many  investigations  have  been  carried  out  on  the 
rate  of  accommodation  of  nerve  and  these  have  shown 
that  accommodation  need  not  be  rapid  and  that  in 
crustacean  (49),  amphibian  (26)  and  mammalian 
(32,  89)  nerve  it  is  in  fact  possible  to  obtain  main- 
tained repetitive  firing  during  the  passage  of  a  con- 
stant current.  Further  it  has  been  found  that  most 
experimental  procedures  tend  to  increase  the  rate  of 
accommodation  (81);  it  is  possible  that  the  common 
eflfect  of  all  these  procedures  is  to  lower  the  membrane 
potential,  a  reduction  of  which  is  known  to  increase 
the  rate  of  accommodation  (94).  These  findings  led 
to  the  view  that  the  mechanism  of  repetitive  firing 
from  sensory  receptors  could  be  explained  on  the 
known  properties  of  nerve  fibers.  This  view  was 
elaborated    in    particular    by    certain    Scandinavian 


workers  (9,  30)  who  suggested  that  the  receptor 
develops  a  'generator  potential'  which  causes  current 
to  flow  in  the  nerve  fiber  so  acting  like  a  constant 
current  stimulus  in  setting  up  a  train  of  impulses. 
This  idea  has  remained  the  basis  of  most  subsequent 
work  on  the  subject. 

The  concept  that  the  inter\als  between  the  im- 
pulses of  a  train  are  dependent  on  the  rate  of  recovery 
after  an  impulse  is  faced  with  the  difficulty  that 
rhythmic  discharges  of  very  low  frequency,  a  few 
impulses  per  second,  can  be  obser\ed.  These  intervals 
are  much  longer  than  the  total  duration  of  the  re- 
covery process  as  known  in  nerve.  Investigations  on 
the  repetitive  firing  of  crustacean  nerve  during  the 
passage  of  a  constant  current  have  introduced  another 
idea  C49)>  that  the  intervals  between  impulses  are 
determined  by  the  response  time.  That  is  to  say  the 
intervals  are  determined  in  the  same  manner  as  the 
latency  from  the  l:)eginning  of  a  current  stimulus  to 
the  initiation  of  the  first  impulse. 

The  passage  of  a  constant  current  through  a  crusta- 
cean axon  sets  up  a  repetitive  discharge  as  shown  in 
figure  3.  Several  points  can  be  seen  in  this  figure;  the 
frequency  of  discharge  is  related  to  the  current 
strength;  the  interval  between  the  beginning  of  the 
current  and  the  first  impulse  is  always  closely  related 
to  the  intervals  between  the  other  impulses;  these 
intervals  are  all  dependent  on  the  development  of 
the  local  response,  an  impulse  being  initiated  when- 
ever this  local  response  reaches  the  critical  potential; 
the  critical  potential  at  which  the  impulses  are  set  up 
is  the  same  with  all  but  the  greatest  strengths  of  cur- 
rent and  all  but  the  highest  frequencies  of  impulses. 
Apart  from  this  direct  evidence  that  it  is  the  time 
course  of  the  development  of  the  local  response  that 
sets  the  interval  between  impulses,  the  recovery  time 
of  these  axons  is  such  that  it  cannot  explain  the  fre- 
quencies observed.  These  crustacean  axons  have  long 
response  times  and  can  therefore  give  regular  low 
frequency  discharges. 

The  events  taking  place  in  certain  stretch  receptors 
in  Crustacea  are  very  similar  (27).  A  microelectrode 
in  the  cell  body  of  one  of  these  primary  sensory  neu- 
rons is  able  to  detect  a  receptor  potential  generated  in 
the  terminals  and,  superimposed  on  it,  a  discharge  of 
nerve  impulses.  The  receptor  potential  will  be  con- 
sidered in  a  later  section.  Here  it  is  sufficient  to  point 
out  that  after  an  impulse  the  membrane  potential 
builds  up  again  in  a  manner  very  similar  to  that 
shown  in  figure  3,  and  the  next  impulse  is  set  up 
when  this  potential  reaches  the  critical  value.  The 


121: 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


50  50 

AAA/WWWW\  AA/\/WWWW\=^^/"" 


eye./ 


FIG.  3.  Responses  at  the  cathode  of  single  carcinus  a,\ons  to 
constant  currents.  A-H:  increasing  currents  as  indicated.  /-A  .■ 
near  threshold  currents  at  higher  amplification  and  faster  sweep 
speed.  L.  potential  change  at  anode,  conditions  as  /-A.  [From 
Hodgkin  (49).] 


critical   potential   remains   the   same   at   all   but   the 
highest  values  of  impulse  frequenc\ . 

The  initiation  of  impulses  b\  the  receptor  potential 
generated  in  the  muscle  spindle  of  the  frog  has  also 
been  observed  (58).  In  this  preparation  the  critical 
potential  remains  constant  for  all  except  the  first  im- 
pulse in  a  discharge  at  a  constant  frequency,  but  the 
value  is  different  for  different  frequencies;  in  fact 
there  is  a  direct  and  linear  relationship  between  the 
value  of  the  critical  potential  and  the  frequency  of 
the  discharge.  An  explanation  of  this  phenomenon 
has  been  given  as  follows  (58) :  recovery  after  a  nerve 
impulse  depends  on  two  processes  o)  a  restoration  of 
membrane  resistance  and  6)  a  return  of  excitability 
(see  48).  If  the  first  of  these  processes  is  the  more 
rapid  in  the  frog  muscle  spindle  fibers,  but  not  in 
the  crustacean  fiber,  then  results  such  as  have  been 
observed  would  be  expected. 


Modifuation  0/  Afferent  Discharges  by  Current 

Afferent  discharges  can  be  modified  by  the  applica- 
tion of  currents  to  the  regions  in  which  such  dis- 
charges are  set  up.  This  can  be  seen  in  the  frog's 
muscle  spindle  (25);  if  the  spindle  is  made  to  dis- 
charge at  a  suitable  frequency  by  stretch  and  a  current 
is  applied  between  an  electrode  on  the  afferent  nerve 
and  another  on  the  muscle,  the  frequency  is  increased 
if  the  electrode  on  the  muscle  is  the  cathode  and  de- 
creased if  this  electrode  is  the  anode.  The  increase  or 
decrease  of  frequency  is  related  to  the  intensity  of 
current,  though  the  relation  is  not  a  simple  one.  Other 
preparations  exhibit  similar  effects.  Current  passed 
through  the  nerve  terminals  of  the  isolated  labyrinth 
of  the  rav  causes  an  increase  in  the  frequency  ot 
discharge  in  these  fibers  when  the  cathode  is  on  the 
tissue  surrounding  the  .sensory  endings  and  the  anode 
on  the  afferent  nerve  fibers;  a  current  in  the  opposite 
direction  causes  a  reduction  in  the  discharge  fre- 
quency (71).  These  changes  caused  by  the  flow  of 
current  summate  with  those  due  to  angular  accelera- 
tion in  the  appropriate  direction.  Similar  results  can 
be  observed  by  polarization  of  the  lateralis  organs  of 
Xenopus  laevis.  It  has  been  shown  that  when  the  applied 
current  flows  along  the  nerve  fiber,  as  in  the  instances 
already  described,  an  increase  in  frequency  occurs 
when  the  cathode  is  on  the  terminal  and  the  anode  on 
the  nerve;  however,  if  the  current  flows  between  elec- 
trodes placed  on  either  side  of  the  skin,  the  frequency 
is  increased  when  the  cathode  is  on  the  inside  and  the 
anode  on  the  outside  (77).  The  frequency  of  discharge 
in  the  nerve  fibers  from  the  lateral  line  organ  of  the 
Japanese  eel  is  also  increased  by  a  current  passed 
between  an  anode  on  the  outside  of  the  skin  and  a 
cathode  on  the  inside  (55);  the  passage  of  a  current  in 
the  same  direction  has  been  shown  to  excite  afferent 
fibers  from  touch  receptors  in  frog  skin  (73).  Currents 
can  al.so  modifv  the  discharge  from  a  compound  eye 
(40). 

These  results  are  important  in  two  respects.  First, 
depolarization  of  the  terminal  parts  of  the  axon 
membrane  can  summate  with  end  organ  activity 
which  suggests  that  the  latter  involves  a  depolariza- 
tion of  the  terminals.  This  is  in  fact  known  to  occur 
in  manv  instances  which  will  be  considered  below. 
Second,  it  can  be  argued  from  the  results  obtained 
with  currents  pas.sed  across  the  skin  instead  of  along 
the  ner\e  that,  during  sensory  activity,  impulses  are 
initiated  away  from  the  terminal  (77)-  Direct  evi- 
dence that  this  is  so  in  certain  instances  will  be  given 
later. 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


129 


EXCITATION    OF    IMPULSES   BY    CONTROLLED    PULSES 
AND   PHASIC   RECEPTORS 

In  the  last  section  stimulus-frequency  relations 
were  considered.  Such  relations  give  important  in- 
formation about  units  that  signal  the  values  of  steady 
states  by  indicating  them  as  particular  and  repeatable 
frequencies  of  impulses.  That  is  to  say  these  relations 
are  important  for  nonadapting  or  tonic  units.  On  the 
other  hand,  the  response  of  phasic  units,  and  the 
adapting  part  of  responses  of  tonic  units,  are  de- 
pendent on  the  time  course  of  the  stimulus;  in  partic- 
ular the  rates  of  change  at  the  beginning  and  end  of 
the  pulse  are  important.  To  investigate  these  phasic 
units  in  detail,  it  is  therefore  important  to  use  stimuli 
of  known  time  course.  It  is  also  important  that  the 
stimulus  should  be  adequately  damped.  The  im- 
portance of  this  can  be  shown  by  an  example:  Pacin- 
ian corpuscles  have  thresholds  of  a  few  tenths  of  a 
micron  and,  for  the  amplitude  threshold  to  be  mini- 
mal, the  displacement  must  be  complete  in  less  than 
a  millisecond  (34);  if  large  displacements  of  tens  of 
microns  are  used,  it  only  requires  a  one  per  cent  oscil- 
lation to  give  rise  to  what  appears  to  be  a  repetitive 
response.  Various  techniques  have  been  used  for  this 
purpose.  Thus  for  mechanical  receptors,  electro- 
magnetic (6,  57)  and  crystal  transducers  (34,  35) 
have  been  used.  The  former  have  bigger  displace- 
ments, but  generally  have  a  slower  time  course  than 
the  latter  which  can  have  a  damped  rise  time  of  0.2 
msec,  and  a  displacement  of  10  to  20  /j.  It  should  be 
noted  that  even  0.2  msec,  is  not  very  short  compared 
with  the  latency  from  the  beginning  of  the  stimulus 
to  the  impulse. 

Quantitative  Aspects  of  Excitation 

Using  such  methods,  the  latencies  for  impulse  ini- 
tiation in  Pacinian  corpuscles  and  frog  skin  receptors 
have  been  measured  (34,  35).  In  the  Paciniaii  cor- 
puscle latencies  after  the  onset  of  mechanical  deforma- 
tions of  any  duration  are  longer  (i.e.  0.5  to  3.0  msec.) 
than  those  following  the  beginning  of  a  constant 
current  stimulus  to  the  receptor's  own  nerve  fiber 
within  a  millimeter  of  the  ending.  After  mechanical 
stimulation  of  frog  skin  even  longer  latencies  have 
been  observed.  The  latency  observed  in  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  can  be  shown  to  be  due  to  the  time  taken 
for  the  receptor  potential  to  develop  (37);  it  seems 
likely,  therefore,  that  the  longer  latencies  found  with 
frog  skin  receptors  indicate  even  more  prolonged 
receptor  proces.ses.  Curves  of  recovery  after  the  ini- 


tiation of  an  impulse  by  a  short  mechanical  pulse  to 
a  Pacinian  corpuscle  have  been  shown  to  be  similar 
to  the  curves  of  recovery  obtained  after  electrical 
excitation  of  the  ending's  own  nerve  fiber  close  to  the 
corpuscle  and  of  nerves  in  general  (34).  Thus,  in  this 
instance  at  least,  there  is  direct  evidence  that  the 
time  course  of  recovery  at  the  site  of  impulse  initiation 
is  not  much  different  from  that  in  other  parts  of 
nerves. 

The  change  of  amplitude  threshold  with  change  of 
stimulus  velocity  has  also  been  measured,  and  the 
minimum  velocity  of  stimulus  necessary  for  excitation 
found.  Thus,  just  as  there  is  a  critical  slope  in  the 
excitation  of  nerve  by  a  linearly  increasing  current, 
so  there  is  a  critical  slope  in  the  excitation  of  phasic 
receptors  by  linearly  increasing  displacements.  Such 
measurements  give  a  quantitative  measure  of  the 
adaptation  of  such  receptors.  Thus  the  critical  slope 
for  a  Pacinian  corpuscle  is  given  as  1 200  rheobases 
per  sec.  (36)  and  that  for  receptors  in  frog's  skin  61 
rheobases  per  sec  (35). 

As  a  means  of  investigating  the  fundamental  mech- 
anisms of  receptors,  such  measurements  have  been 
superseded  by  direct  recording  of  receptor  potentials; 
but  they  are  still  of  use  in  certain  types  of  quantitative 
investigation  (53). 

On  and  Off  Responses 

At  least  some  phasic  receptors  respond  with  one  or 
a  few  impulses  to  a  change  from  one  state  to  another; 
this  response  is  not  qualitatively  dependent  on  the 
sign  of  this  change.  Thus  many  photoreceptors  re- 
spond when  the  intensity  of  illumination  on  them  is 
suddenly  raised  from  one  level  to  another  and  again 
when  the  intensity  is  suddenly  reduced  (30).  The  same 
type  of  response  to  change  of  state  is  seen  in  receptors 
in  toad  and  cat  skin  (73).  Measurements  of  the 
threshold  amplitude  for  on  and  oft"  responses  to  rec- 
tangular displacements  have  been  made  for  Pacinian 
corpuscles  and  frog  skin  receptors;  in  the  former  the 
threshold  for  a  compression  (the  'on  response')  is 
usually  slightly  lower  than  that  for  a  decompression 
(the  'off  response'),  but  not  infrequently  the  reverse  is 
true  (34);  on  the  other  hand  the  excitability  of  the 
frog's  cutaneous  receptors  to  a  compression  is  much 
greater  than  the  excitability  to  the  decompression 
(35).  These  difTerences  may  well  be  due  to  the 
mechanics  of  the  systems,  for  in  these  experiments 
compression  is  a  result  of  an  externally  applied  force, 
while  decompression  depends  solely  on  the  restoring 
forces  inherent  in  the  tissue;  it  is  likely  that  restoration 


I30 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NELROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


is  a  much  more  rapid  process  in  a  Pacinian  corpuscle 
than  in  frog's  skin. 

Summatiim 

Two  subthreshold  short  pulses  applied  to  a  phasic 
receptor  within  a  suitable  interval  of  each  other  can 
summate  and  set  up  an  impulse;  the  essential  point 
in  this  experiment  is  that  the  first  pulse  is  over  before 
the  beginning  of  the  second  and  the  summation  takes 
place  in  the  receptor.  Further  discussion  of  this  point 
will  be  left  to  the  ne.xt  section  where  receptor  poten- 
tials are  discussed.  One  particular  case  can,  however, 
be  discussed  here.  It  is  possible  to  observe  summation 
between  the  subthreshold  activity  evoked  by  a  small 
short  mechanical  pulse  and  a  brief  electrical  test 
shock.  .Such  a  test  shock  can  be  used  to  measure  the 
excitability  of  the  receptor  at  different  times  after  the 
application  of  the  mechanical  pulse;  in  this  way  in- 
direct evidence  of  the  time  course  of  a  receptor  poten- 
tial has  been  obtained  (34)- 


RECEPTOR  POTENTIALS  AND  OTHER 
GENERATOR  POTENTIALS 

It  is  now  widely  held  that  the  immediate  cause  of 
impulse  initiation  in  receptors  and  sense  organs  is  the 
development  of  an  electrical  potential  change  which  is 
graded  according  to  certain  characteristics  of  the 
stimulus  and  which  is  confined  to  the  region  of  the 
receptor  or  organ.  .Such  potentials  have  now  been 
found  in  a  number  of  situations  of  different  types  and 
these  findings,  together  with  supporting  evidence  such 
as  summation  results  from  other  sites,  form  the  justi- 
fication for  such  a  generalization. 

In  this  .section  I  shall  use  the  term  'generator  po- 
tential' to  describe  any  graded  potential  change  oc- 
curring in  a  sensory  receptor  or  in  a  complex  sense 
organ  that  can  reasonably  be  supposed  to  be  a  cause 
of  the  initiation  of  an  impulse.  The  term  'receptor 
potentials'  I  will  confine  to  those  generator  potentials 
occurring  in  a  single  receptor.  Thus  the  cochlea 
microphonic  is  a  generator  potential  but  not  a  re- 
ceptor potential. 

Generator  Potentials  in  Complex  Organs 

These  lie  outside  the  scope  of  this  particular  chapter 
but  are  included  briefly  for  completeness.  The  cochlea 
is  the  best  example  of  this  group.  In  this  organ  there  is 
a  potential  difference  maintained  between  the  endo- 


lymph  and  the  perilymph  (96).  During  the  application 
of  a  sound  wave,  an  alternating  potential  can  be  re- 
corded and  shown  to  have  its  greatest  intensity  at  the 
point  on  the  basilar  membrane  at  which  the  hair  cells 
are  situated  (16).  This  potential  is  directly  related  to 
the  sound  pressure  wave  (100).  There  is  reason  to 
suppose  that  this  microphonic  potential,  as  it  is  called, 
is  the  cause  of  impulse  initiation  (16).  'Microphonic' 
potentials  have  also  been  found  in  other  sites,  e.g. 
the  lateral  line  organs  (54)  and  sacculus  (105).  These 
potentials  serve  a  similar  function  to  the  receptor 
potentials  of  neurons  but,  in  the  cochlea  at  least, 
they  represent  changes  of  potential  between  multi- 
cellular compartments  instead  of  across  cell  mem- 
branes. It  is  not  improbable  that  there  are  common 
factors  in  the  development  of  these  two  types  of  po- 
tential, but  we  cannot  expect  to  find  close  parallels. 

Receptor  Potentials  Generated  in  Nerve  Terminals 

.Such  potentials  have  been  recorded  from  certain 
mechanically  excitable  receptors  (6,  27,  37,  58},  from 
photoreceptors  (42,  79)  and  from  olfactory  receptors 
(78).  In  all  these  instances  the  receptor  potential  has 
been  recorded  at  a  distance  from  its  source,  and  in  no 
case  has  the  membrane  potential  of  the  receptor 
region  been  recorded  directly.  In  each  of  the  three 
mechanical  examples  on  which  we  have  information 
at  present,  the  records  were  obtained  by  recording 
the  currents  flowing  along  the  nerve  fiber,  the  nerve 
fiber  behaving  as  a  pair  of  passive  concentric  conduc- 
tors. The  changes  in  these  currents  must  have  been 
related  to  changes  in  potential  across  the  membrane 
of  the  terminal  portions  of  the  afferent  nerve  fiber, 
since  all  currents  recorded  must  have  crossed  the 
membrane  peripheral  to  the  recording  region;  this 
does  not  prove  of  course  that  the  changes  are  actively 
generated  across  the  terminal  membrane.  Reasons  for 
believing  that  the  receptor  potentials  are  in  fact 
actively  generated  at  this  site  are  given  in  the  last 
section  of  this  chapter. 

Examples  of  receptor  potentials  are  shown  in 
figure  4.  Figure  4.-1  and  B  are  records  from  muscle 
spindles  from  the  frog  (58);  in  both  experiments  the 
preparations  had  been  procainized  to  prevent  impulse 
activity.  Figure  ^A  shows  the  changes  that  occur  at 
the  beginning  of  a  maintained  stretch;  it  can  be  seen 
that  there  is  a  relatively  large  initial  change  of  poten- 
tial and  that  this  is  foOowed  by  a  small  but  main- 
tained potential  change.  The  earlier  phase,  called  the 
dynamic  phase,  is  related  to  the  velocity  of  the 
stretch.  The  smaller  maintained  change  of  potential 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


'S' 


FIG.  4.  Receptor  potentials  from  different  receptors.  .1  and  B.  from  frogs  muscle  spindle,  pro- 
cainized.  Top:  stretch.  Bottom:  receptor  potential.  Time,  A,  500  cps;  B,  o.i  sec.  [From  Katz  (58).] 
C:  from  cat's  Pacinian  corpuscle,  procainized.  Upper  trace  {at  starty  amplitude  and  duration  of 
displacement  and  time  in  msec.  Note  that  this  trace  crosses  the  other  trace  during  displacement. 
Lower  trace  (at  starty  receptor  potential  record.  [From  Gray  &  Sato  (37).]  D:  from  crayfish 
stretch  receptor.  Arrows  mark  duration  of  stretch.  Time,  i  sec.  [From  Eyzaguirre  &  Kuffler  (27).] 


depends  only  on  the  amplitude  of  the  stretch.  Figure 
4B  shows  also  the  events  occurring  when  the  stretch  is 
released.  It  can  be  seen  that  there  is  a  change  of 
potential  in  the  opposite  direction  to  the  other  deflec- 
tions, that  is  the  electrode  near  the  receptor  goes  posi- 
ti\e  to  the  distant  electrode.  The  tiine  course  of  this 
deflection  tends  to  be  slower  than  that  of  the  initial 
dynamic  phase,  but  it  must  be  remembered  that 
relaxation  of  the  muscle  depends  on  the  restoring 
forces  in  the  tissue  while  the  stretch  is  actively  im- 
posed. The  three  phases  of  the  receptor  potential 
correspond  to  the  initial  burst,  to  the  maintained 
discharge  and  to  the  reduced  discharge  that  follows 
the  end  of  a  stretch. 

Figure  4C  (37)  shows  a  receptor  potential  from  a 
Pacinian  corpuscle  and  with  it  the  voltage  pulse  ap- 
plied to  the  crystal  transducer  that  was  used  to  stimu- 
late; impulse  activity  has  been  prevented  with  pro- 
caine. This  potential  differs  in  several  respects  from 
that  found  in  the  muscle  spindle.  There  is  no  main- 
tained plateau,  the  potential  declining  to  zero  once 
the  peak  is  past.  The  shape  of  the  receptor  potential 
is  nearly  or  completely  the  same  whether  excited  by 
a  short  pulse  of  say  0.3  msec,  duration,  Ijy  the  be- 


ginning of  a  long  pulse  (fig.  4C)  or  by  the  end  of  a 
long  pulse.  As  in  the  case  of  the  muscle  spindle  these 
results  are  consistent  with  the  results  of  experiments 
on  the  excitation  of  impulses  by  these  receptors.  These 
two  examples  illustrate  contrasting  types  of  receptor 
potential,  the  one  associated  with  tonic  behavior  and 
the  other  with  phasic  behavior.  In  particular  it  is 
worth  noting  that  a  receptor  potential,  and  with  it  an 
impulse,  is  set  up  by  decompression  of  a  Pacinian  cor- 
puscle, while  relaxation  of  a  muscle  spindle  is  asso- 
ciated with  a  positive  going  receptor  potential  and 
an  inhibition  of  the  impulse  discharge. 

The  receptor  potential  in  figure  4Z)  is  that  of  a 
slowly  adapting  stretch  receptor  from  the  crayfish 
(27).  This  was  recorded  by  means  of  a  microelectrode 
in  the  cell  body  of  the  neuron  which,  in  this  instance, 
lies  in  the  periphery  close  to  the  muscle;  the  receptor 
part  of  the  cell  lies  still  further  to  the  periphery  in  the 
terminations  that  ramify  in  the  receptor  muscle.  The 
record  shows  a  steady  depolarization  well  maintained 
throughout  the  stretch;  there  is  no  marked  dynamic 
phase,  even  when  the  early  part  of  the  potential  is  not 
obscured  by  spikes,  as  in  figure  4Z),  though  there  is 
soine  initial  decline  in  the  level  of  the  depolarization; 


'32 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


at  the  end  of  the  stretch  the  potential  simply  returns 
to  its  resting  level.  This  difference  from  the  muscle 
spindle  potentials  may  be  due  to  the  effective  velocity 
of  the  stretch.  In  this  record,  unlike  those  that  are 
illustrated  in  figure  ^A,  B  and  C,  the  impulse  dis- 
charge has  not  been  interfered  with  and  five  spikes 
are  shown  arising  from  the  receptor  potential.  It  can 
be  seen  that  each  impulse  is  preceded  by  a  relatively 
slow  decrease  in  membrane  potential  (the  prepoten- 
tial)  and  that  when  this  decrease  reaches  a  critical 
value  the  impulse  is  discharged. 

Receptor  potentials  are  not  confined  to  mechani- 
cally excited  receptors.  It  has  long  been  known  that 
slow  potentials  could  be  obtained  from  the  retina  and 
from  compound  eyes  (31).  There  has  been  reason  to 
suppose  that  part  at  least  of  these  potentials  repre- 
sented activity  of  the  receptors  themselves.  Direct 
evidence  that  single  ommatidia  produce  receptor 
potentials  has  now  been  obtained.  In  the  single 
isolated  ommatidium  of  Limulus  (42)  a  receptor 
potential  builds  up  rapidly  when  the  ommatidium 
is  illuminated.  The  potential  then  dies  away,  but 
with  suitable  recording  conditions  some  depolari- 
zation appears  to  remain  as  long  as  the  receptor  is 
illuminated.  The  cessation  of  illumination  is  not 
accompanied  by  hyperpolarization.  The  olfactory 
mucosa  of  the  frog  produces  slow  potential  changes 
when  excited  by  air  containing  a  suitable  agent  (78). 
The  distribution  in  area  and  in  depth  of  these 
potentials  and  their  relative  insensitivity  to  cocaine 
suggest  that  they  are  due  to  synchronous  activity  of 
the  olfactory  receptors. 

Relation  of  Receptor  Potentials  to  Impulse  Initiation 

There  can  be  little  doubt  that  receptor  potentials 
are  the  immediate  cause  of  the  initiation  of  impulses. 
They  always  precede  the  impulse  and  the  impulses 
appear  when  a  critical  potential  has  been  reached. 
In  the  crustacean  stretch  receptor  this  critical  level 
remains  constant  under  a  variety  of  conditions.  With 
the  frog's  muscle  spindle  the  critical  potential  depends 
on  the  frequency  of  the  discharge,  an  observation 
which  has  been  discussed  above.  In  this  preparation 
the  frequency  of  discharge  is  linearly  related  to  the 
amplitude  of  the  receptor  potential,  a  fact  which  sug- 
gests that  the  receptor  potential  is  causally  related  to 
the  impulse  discharge.  That  such  a  relationship  is  not 
immediately  visible  in  the  results  obtained  from  the 
crustacean  stretch  receptor  does  not  mean,  of  course, 
that  the  frequency  of  the  impulse  discharge  is  not 
related   to  the  amplitude  of  the  receptor  potential. 


This  is  perhaps  best  explained  by  considering  the 
steps  involved  in  the  initiation  of  the  impulse.  There 
are  reasons,  which  will  be  considered  below,  for  sup- 
posing that  the  impulses  are  initiated  at  a  point  which 
is  near  but  not  identical  with  that  at  which  the  re- 
ceptor potential  is  generated.  Currents  due  to  the 
receptor  potential  will  then  flow  through  and  dis- 
charge the  membrane  of  the  neighboring  parts  of  the 
nerve  fiber;  this  part  of  the  membrane  will  develop 
local  responses  (48,  56),  and  if  the  membrane  poten- 
tial falls  to  the  critical  level  an  impulse  will  he 
discharged.  This  sequence  of  events  is  essentially  the 
same  as  that  found  during  the  repetitive  firing  of  a 
carcinus  axon  in  response  to  an  externally  applied 
constant  current  (49).  The  slowly  rising  prepotentials 
of  the  crayfish  stretch  receptor  (fig.  4Z))  are  similar 
to  those  of  the  current  excited  carcinus  axon  (fig.  3). 
In  both  these  examples  the  recording  conditions  are 
such  that  what  is  recorded  is  related  to  the  membrane 
potential  at  the  point  of  impulse  initiation  and  not 
to  the  intensity  of  the  charging  current  or  the  po- 
tential of  the  source  supplying  this  current.  In  conse- 
quence what  is  seen  is  the  passive  discharging  and 
local  response  of  the  membrane  at  the  site  of  initiation 
followed  by  the  impulse  if  and  when  the  memlDrane 
potential  falls  to  a  critical  level;  this  part  of  the 
membrane  is  then  repolarized  and  the  cycle  starts 
again.  The  rate  of  discharging  of  the  membrane  and 
hence  the  frequency  of  the  impulses  depends  on  the 
intensity  of  the  discharging  current  which  in  turn 
depends  on  the  size  of  the  receptor  potential;  this, 
however,  is  masked  during  a  train  of  impulses.  If  the 
amplitude  of  the  receptor  potential  could  be  measured 
during  the  impulse  discharge  a  relationship  between 
receptor  potential  amplitude  and  frequency  would 
no  doubt  be  found,  and  this  might  be  similar  to  the 
relation  Ijetween  applied  current  and  frequency  in 
the  carcinus  axon.  A  relation  was  found  in  the  case  of 
the  frog's  muscle  spindle  because,  between  impulses, 
conditions  were  such  that  the  full  amplitude  of  the 
receptor  potential  was  recorded;  this  was  proved  by 
subsequent  procainization.  Possible  reasons  for  this 
behavior  have  already  been  considered. 

Qjiantitatwe  Relations  Between  Stimulus  and 
Receptor  Potential 

The  amplitudes  of  the  receptor  potentials  of  the 
muscle  spindle  and  Pacinian  corpuscle  increase  with 
the  amplitude  of  the  displacement  up  to  a  certain 
point  and  then  level  off  to  a  maximum.  An  example  is 
shown  in  figure  5.  This  particular  example  was  ob- 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


'33 


100  r 
% 
90 


70 
60 
SO 

■10 
)0 
20  - 
10 


F19.5 


Stiinulus  scrength 


10 
1 


Fig    6 


o    O 


oo 


10 
2 


0 

0      0 
0  0 

0        0 
0 

0° 

Stimului  strength 

IS                        20 

25 

30 

3                       4 

S 

« 

FIG.  5.  Receptor  potential  amplitude  in  relation  to  the  displacement  of  the  mechanical  stimulus 
with  velocity  constant  in  a  Pacinian  corpuscle.  Abscissa:  stimulus  strength  in  arbitrary  units.  Ordi- 
nate: receptor  potential  amplitude  as  percentage  of  maximum.  O  same  points  as  •,  but  stimulus 
strength  scale  expanded  five  times.  [From  Gray  &  Sato  (37).^ 

FIG.  6.  Receptor  potential  rate  of  rise  in  relation  to  the  displacement  of  the  mechanical  stimulus 
with  velocity  constant  in  a  Pacinian  corpuscle.  Abscissa:  stimulus  strength  in  arbitrary  units.  Ordi- 
nate: receptor  potential  rate  of  rise  as  percentages  of  maximum  amplitude  per  msec.  O  same  points 
as  •,  but  stimulus  strength  scale  expanded  five  times.  [From  Gray  &  Sato  (37).] 


tained  from  a  Pacinian  corpuscle  (37),  but  a  siinilar 
relationship  has  been  observed  in  the  muscle  spindle 
of  the  frog  (58}.  The  value  of  the  receptor  potential 
reaches  a  constant  level  with  large  displacements;  this 
is  not  conclusively  proved  by  the  published  data, 
partly  because  of  the  limits  to  the  size  of  stimulus  used 
and  partly,  in  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  experiments, 
because  the  biggest  stimuli  introduced  artifacts  that 
tended  to  sum  with  the  response.  The  question  of  the 
absolute  amplitude  of  this  ma.ximum  is  considered  in 
a  later  section. 

The  rate  of  rise  of  the  potential  is  also  related 
to  the  size  of  the  exciting  displacement  (37).  This  is 
shown  in  figure  6.  There  is  a  change  in  the  slope  of 
this  graph  at  that  level  of  stimulus  strength  above 
which  the  amplitude  increase  is  limited,  but  even 
above  this  point  the  rate  of  rise  of  the  potential  con- 
tinues to  increase  with  stimulus  strength.  That  this  is 
a  genuine  effect  is  supported  h\  the  fact  that  the  time 
of  rise  of  the  potential  continues  to  shorten  over  this 
range  of  stimuli.  Since  the  recorded  potential  is  a 
result  of  a  potential  change  across  the  terminal  mem- 
brane (whether  or  not  the  potential  is  actively  gen- 
erated at  this  site),  the  rate  of  rise  of  the  receptor 
potential  will  reflect  the  rate  at  which  current  flows 
in  to  the  capacity  of  this  membrane.  In  other  words 
these  results  suggest  that  the  current  across  the  mem- 
brane of  the  nerve  fiber  terminal  continues  to  increase 


as  the  stimulus  increases  even  though  the  peak  po- 
tential has  reached  a  maximum  \alue. 

The  amplitude  of  certain  receptor  potentials,  for 
example  those  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  and  the 
early  phase  of  that  of  the  frog's  muscle  spindle  (fig. 
\A),  is  also  dependent  on  the  velocity  of  the  displace- 
ment. Indirect  evidence  shows  that  this  is  also  true  of 
other  receptors  responding  to  other  forms  of  energy, 
for  example  thermal  receptors  (46).  Figure  7  illus- 
trates the  change  in  relati\e  ainplitude  of  the  receptor 
potential  that  accompanies  change  in  the  velocity  of 
the  mechanical  stimulus,  the  amplitude  of  the  stimu- 
lus ijeing  kept  constant.  It  is  immediately  clear  that 
the  amplitude  of  the  receptor  potential,  while  inde- 
pendent of  velocity  at  high  values,  is  over  a  certain 
range  closely  related  to  the  velocity  of  the  stimulus. 
The  'angle'  of  this  curve  occurs,  for  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle,  at  a  compression  velocity  of  about  i  mm 
per  sec.  (i.e.  about  5  thresholds  per  msec).  This 
means  that  many  physiological  stimuli  may  be  ex- 
pected to  lie  within  the  velocity-sensitive  range. 

The  time  course  of  the  receptor  potential  is  in  most 
instances  dependent  on  the  properties  of  the  stimulus. 
It  has  already  been  pointed  out  that  the  rate  of  rise 
of  the  potential  \aries  with  stimulus  amplitude  and 
this  change  in  the  rate  of  rise  of  the  potential  change 
is  accompanied  by  a  change  in  the  time  of  rise  (37, 
58).  The  rate  of  rise  of  the  potential  change  may  also 
be    affected    by    the    \elocity   (or   comparable    time 


'34 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


100 

% 

K 
00 
70 
M 
SO 
40 
M 
20 
10 
0 


••     • 


Stimulus  velocity  (V/miec) 
_I I 


FIG.  J.  Receptor  potential  amplituide  in  relation  to  the 
velocity  of  the  mechanical  stimulus  with  displacement  constant 
in  a  Pacinian  corpuscle.  Abscissa/  stimulus  velocity  in  arbitrary 
units.  Ordinate:  receptor  potential  amplitutie  as  percentage  of 
maximum.   [From  Gray  &   Sato  (37).] 


function  of  the  relevant  form  of  energy)  of  the  pulse 
used  to  excite;  thus  the  rate  of  rise  of  the  receptor 
potential  of  the  frog's  muscle  spindle  gets  less  as  the 
velocity  of  the  stimulus  is  reduced.  In  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  there  may  Ije  some  change  in  rate  of  rise, 
but  often  there  is  no  effect  attributable  specifically  to 
the  stimulus  velocity;  that  is  to  say  that,  though  the 
rate  of  rise  of  the  potential  change  increases  as  its 
amplitude  increases,  the  time  course  of  a  receptor 
potential  of  a  given  amplitude  is  often  the  same 
whether  it  is  produced  by  a  small  displacement 
having  a  high  velocity  or  by  a  larger  displacement  of 
lower  velocity.  In  other  words  there  are  many  me- 
chanical pulses  having  different  values  of  amplitude 
and  velocity  that  are  equivalent  as  'stimuli'. 

The  duration  of  static  receptor  potentials,  e.g.  that 
of  the  frog's  muscle  spindle  and  the  slowly  adapting 
stretch  receptor  of  the  crayfish,  is  directly  dependent 
on  the  duration  of  the  applied  force.  The  rate  of  decay 
of  those  potentials,  which  are  velocity  sensitive,  may 
possibly  depend  on  the  duration  of  the  applied  force 
under  certain  circumstances;  however,  the  rate  of 
decay  of  the  receptor  potentials  of  the  Pacinian  cor- 
puscle, the  only  end  organ  in  which  this  particular 
point  has  been  investigated,  is  normally  independent 
of  the  duration  of  the  stimulus  (37).  Off  responses 
have  the  same  time  course  as  on  responses. 

Absolute  Magnitude  oj  the  Receptor  Potential 

Receptor  potentials  reach  a  ma.ximum  at  a  certain 
value  of  stimulus  strength.  It  is  of  considerable  theo- 


retical importance  to  know  the  absolute  value  of  this 
potential  change.  Up  to  the  present,  it  has  been  pos- 
sible to  make  only  a  rough  estimate  of  its  value  in  the 
Pacinian  corpuscle  (20).  This  has  been  done  by  re- 
cording the  external  current  flowing  along  the  axon 
between  the  second  and  third  nodes  of  Ranvier  during 
activity  of  each  of  these  nodes  and  of  the  receptor 
potential.  By  the  use  of  blocking  techniques  and  by 
taking  diff"erences,  the.se  components  were  obtained 
separately  and  measured.  Under  suitable  conditions 
these  currents  will  he  proportional  to  the  driving 
potentials.  The  results  given  are  that  the  receptor 
potential  amplitude  is  59  per  cent  (n  =  6,  S.D.  = 
14  per  cent)  of  the  amplitude  of  the  impulse  at  node  2 
and  38  per  cent  (n  =  5,  S.D.  =  17  per  cent)  of  the 
amplitude  of  the  impulse  at  node  3.  The  difference 
between  the  figures  is  due  to  a  decline  in  the  impulse 
amplitude  as  the  terminal  is  approached.  The  atten- 
uation per  internode  of  the  receptor  potential  is  likely 
to  be  less  than  the  0.5  for  large  myelinated  fibers  of 
toads  C92),  so  the  absolute  value  of  the  receptor 
potential  can  be  considered  as  of  the  same  order  of 
magnitude  as  the  resting  and  action  potentials.  This 
conclusion  is  supported  by  results  from  the  crayfish 
stretch  receptor  (27).  The  amplitude  of  the  recorded 
receptor  potential  at  threshold  ranges  from  8  to  25  mv 
depending  on  the  type  of  receptor.  It  has  been  esti- 
mated that  the  loss  due  to  passive  conduction  along 
the  nerve  filler  will  have  reduced  the  true  value  of  the 
receptor  potential  by  20  to  80  per  cent;  also  a  maxi- 
mum receptor  potential  must  be  appreciably  greater 
than  a  threshold  one.  The  ratio  for  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  is  10  to    i  (37). 


Summation  oj  Receptor  Potentials 

If,  during  a  maintained  receptor  potential,  the  re- 
ceptor is  subjected  to  increase  in  the  stimulus  strength 
the  final  value  of  the  receptor  potential  will  correspond 
to  the  final  value  of  the  stimulus.  In  this  instance  both 
the  stimulus  and  the  receptor  potential  have  summed. 
With  short  pulse  excitation  it  has  been  shown  that 
summation  of  receptor  potentials  occurs  after  the 
stimulus  is  over  (6,  37)  as  shown  in  figure  8.  This 
summation  appears  similar  to  that  found  with  end- 
plate  potentials  and  synaptic  potentials.  A  special  case 
of  summation  occurs  when  an  'on'  response  summates 
with  an  'ofli"  response  (37).  Summation  of  sub- 
threshold receptor  potentials  can  in  this  way  set  up 
impulses  (6)  and  it  seems  likely  that  this  process  is  of 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


135 


FIG.  8.  Summation  of  receptor  potentials  with  different 
intervals  between  stimuli.  Upper  trace:  stimulus  signal  and  time 
in  msec.  Lower  trace:  receptor  potentials.  [From  Gray  &  Sato 
(37)-] 


considerable  functional  importance  in  determining 
maximum  sensitivities.  It  is  probably  also  important 
in  determining  the  thresholds  for  sensation  at  different 
frequencies  of  vibration  (83). 

Depression 

After  the  production  of  one  receptor  potential  by  a 
Pacinian  corpuscle  a  subsequent  one,  occurring  within 
a  few  milliseconds,  is  depressed.  This  is  most  easily 
seen  with  a  preparation  in  which  impulse  activity  has 
been  prevented;  it  can  then  be  seen  that  the  depres- 
sion of  the  test  responses  increases  as  the  conditioning 
stimulus  is  increased  and  decreases  as  the  interval  be- 
tween the  conditioning  and  test  pulses  is  increased 
(18,  37).  Depression  of  the  receptor  potential  is  also 
caused  by  an  impulse  set  up  as  a  result  of  mechanical 
stimulation;  this  depression  is  much  greater  than  that 
produced  by  a  threshold  receptor  potential  alone, 
though  it  does  not  appear  to  be  as  great  as  the  de- 
pression caused  by  really  large  mechanical  stimuli, 
whether  an  impulse  is  present  or  not.  Antidromically 
conducted  impulses  also  cause  depression  of  a  subse- 
quent receptor  potential,  though  for  any  given  time 
interval  after  the  impulse  the  depression  is  slightly  less 


than  when  the  conditioning  impulse  is  excited  me- 
chanically. At  the  time  of  writing  there  are  a  number 
of  problems  which  require  elucidation  and  on  which 
the  e\idence  is  conflicting. 

Depression  has  not  been  described  for  other  recep- 
tor potentials,  but  this  is  not  surprising  as  the  stim- 
ulating conditions  have  been  very  different.  It  would 
be  interesting  to  know,  however,  if  any  part  of  the 
initial  decline  of  other  receptor  potentials  were  due 
to  the  same  cause  as  this  depression;  the  decline  of 
the  Pacinian  corpuscle  potential  appears  to  be  due 
to  other  and  more  rapid  processes. 

SITE  OF  IMPULSE   INITIATION 

There  is  evidence  from  the  rapidly  adapting  type 
of  stretch  receptor  of  the  crayfish  that  impulses  are 
set  up  in  the  cell  body  (27).  The  records  in  these  ex- 
periments were  made  through  an  electrode  that  was 
inside  the  cell  body  and  it  was  found  that  the  change 
of  memijrane  potential  required  to  excite  an  impulse 
was  the  same  whether  this  change  was  brought  about 
by  a  receptor  potential  spreading  from  the  periphery 
or  by  current  spread  from  an  antidromically  con- 
ducted impulse  that  had  been  blocked  before  it 
invaded  the  cell  body.  If  the  receptor  potential  set  up 
impulses  peripheral  to  the  cell  body,  the  apparent 
threshold  value  of  the  receptor  potential,  as  recorded 
by  this  method,  would  be  less  than  the  true  value  by 
the  amount  of  decrement  occurring  between  the  site 
of  initiation  and  the  cell  ijody;  this  is  in  fact  what 
occurs  in  the  slowly  adapting  stretch  receptors  of  the 
same  species.  Direct  distortion  of  the  cell  body  and 
the  larger  dendrites  does  not  produce  any  potential 
changes;  receptor  potentials  are  produced  only  by 
stretching  the  muscle  fibers  in  which  the  finer  termi- 
nals of  the  neuron  ramify.  It  therefore  seems  certain 
that  while  the  impulses  are  initiated  in  the  cell  body 
of  the  rapidly  adapting  receptor,  the  receptor  poten- 
tials are  developed  peripheral  to  this  in  the  finer 
dendritic  terminals. 

A  similar  state  of  affairs  appears  to  occur  in  the 
Pacinian  corpuscle  (20).  In  this  receptor  a  straight 
nonmyelinated  fiber  of  2  pi  diameter  runs  down  the 
central  core  of  the  corupscle;  at  the  end  of  this  central 
core  the  axon  becomes  myelinated.  One  node  of 
Ranvier  is  regularly  found  inside  the  corpuscle  about 
half  way  between  the  end  of  the  central  core  and  the 
point  at  which  the  axon  leaves  the  capsule,  and  the 
second  occurs  near  the  latter  point  (86).  The  imme- 
diate  surroundings   of   the    nonmyelinated    terminal 


.36 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOL(K;V 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


have  been  shown  to  be  specialized  (85),  and  it  must 
be  supposed  that  it  is  in  this  region  that  the  receptor 
potential  is  generated.  By  recording  across  a  barrier 
surrounding  the  internode  between  the  second  and 
third  nodes  of  Ranvier,  it  was  found  possible  to  record 
distinct  phases  of  activity  due  to  each  of  the  first  two 
nodes  if  the  thresholds  of  these  nodes  were  raised  by 
anodal  polarization.  No  phase  of  impulse  activity 
could  be  found  attributalile  to  the  nonmyelinated 
terminal  even  though  thresholds  were  raised  by  an 
amount  that,  on  theoretical  grounds,  should  have 
been  quite  adequate  to  reveal  such  impulse  activity 
if  it  existed.  It  therefore  appears  that  after  a  mechani- 
cal stimulus  the  impulse  is  set  up  at  the  first  node  of 
Ranvier. 

Indirect  evidence  that  impulses  are  not  initiated  in 
the  terminations  of  the  afferent  nerve  fibers  of  certain 
other  preparations  has  already  been  considered  in  the 
section  on  the  effects  of  applied  currents. 

Not  only  is  there  evidence  that  impulses  are,  in 
some  receptors  at  least,  set  up  away  from  the  termi- 
nals in  which  the  receptor  potentials  are  generated, 
but  there  is  also  evidence  that  such  terminals  are  not 
invaded  by  antidromically  conducted  impulses.  In 
the  crayfish  stretch  receptor  the  receptor  potential 
is  not  abolished  by  an  antidromic  action  potential;  if 
the  impulse  invaded  the  membrane  that  is  involved 
in  the  production  of  the  receptor  potential  one  would 
expect  a  complete  short  circuiting  of  this  membrane 
and  the  temporary  abolition  of  the  receptor  potential 
(28).  A  similar  observation  has  been  made  with  the 
olfactory  mucous  membrane  of  the  frog  (78);  stimula- 
tion of  the  olfactory  nerve  at  different  strengths  and 
frequencies  had  no  effect  on  the  response  of  the  olfac- 
torv  membrane  to  an  exciting  .substance.  As  has 
already  been  stated  in  the  last  section,  an  antidromic 
impulse  causes  slightly  less  depression  of  the  receptor 
potential  in  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  than  does  an 
impulse  set  up  by  a  mechanical  pulse.  It  has  already 
been  argued  that  an  impulse  initiated  in  this  receptor 
by  a  mechanical  stimulus  starts  at  the  first  node  of 
Ranvier;  if  an  antidromically  conducted  impulse  in- 
vaded the  nonmyelinated  terminal  then  it  would  be 
expected  to  produce  a  greater  depression  of  the 
receptor  potential.  This  is  not  the  case  and  it  seems, 
therefore,  that  antidromic  impulses  do  not  invade  the 
nonmyelinated  terminal  (18). 

Evidence  that  impulses  set  up  by  physiological 
stimuli  to  receptors  do  not  start  in  the  receptor  region 
might  simply  mean  that  all-or-nothing  impulses  can- 
not occur  there  during  receptor  activity.  That  anti- 
dromic impulses  do  not  invade  the  terminals  might  be 


a  result  of  Ijlock  at  regions  of  low  safety  factor,  though 
from  parallel  situations  elsewhere  this  does  not  seem 
ver)  likely.  The  most  probable  explanation  of  all 
these  results  is  that  those  regions  of  membrane  that 
are  not  in\aded  are  different  from  the  rest  of  the 
neuron  surface  and  are  not  capable  of  producing  a 
regenerative  all-or-nothing  response. 

When  a  frog's  muscle  spindle  is  discharging  at  low 
frequency  small  all-or-nothing  potentials  can  be  seen. 
These  are  much  smaller  than  the  propagated  impulse 
and  may  occur  in  a  number  of  discrete  sizes  (57). 
They  disappear  if  the  frequency  of  discharge  of  full- 
size  impulses  is  increased  and  also  if  the  receptor  is 
bombarded  antidromically.  After  a  full-size  impulse 
there  is  always  a  delay  before  the  next  all-or-nothing 
event,  whether  full-size  or  small,  but  after  one  of  the 
small  all-or-nothing  potentials  the  interval  may  be 
quite  short.  An  explanation  of  these  events  may  be 
that  impulses  are  set  up  in  the  terminal  branches  of 
this  type  of  receptor,  but  an  impulse  in  a  single  branch 
is  unable  to  pass  the  regions  of  low  safety  factor  that 
occur  where  the  branches  join  (57).  A  full-size  im- 
pulse would  then  only  be  set  up  if  there  were  sufficient 
synchrony  in  the  activity  of  the  terminal  branches. 
On  the  same  argument  all-or-nothing  activity  in  a 
single  branch  would  fail  to  invade  other  branches  and 
therefore  would  not  depress  their  activity,  while  a 
full-size  antidromic  impulse  would  invade  them  all. 


EFFECT   OF   PROC.MNE   .J^ND   SODIUM   LACK   ON 
RECEPTOR    POTENTIALS 

In  the  frog's  muscle  spindle  concentrations  of  pro- 
caine from  0.1  to  0.3  per  cent  abolish  impulse  acti\ity 
but  leave  the  receptor  potential  apparently  unaffected. 
Higher  concentrations  of  procaine  reduce  the  ampli- 
tude of  the  receptor  potential,  affecting  the  static 
phase  more  than  the  dynamic  (58).  Similar  results 
can  be  obtained  with  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  of  the 
cat.  The  impulse  is  abolished  by  concentrations  of 
o.i  to  0.5  per  cent  procaine  in  the  bathing  fluid,  but 
if  the  procaine  is  washed  out  after  aijout  10  min. 
there  is  no  reduction  in  the  amplitude  of  the  receptor 
potential.  Prolonged  soaking  in  these  concentrations 
causes  a  reduction  of  the  receptor  potential  amplitude 

(37)- 

Similar  eff"ects  can  be  obtained  in  both  these  prep- 
arations if  they  are  soaked  in  sodium-free  solutions. 
Ten  minutes  .soaking  in  such  a  solution  abolishes 
repetitive  firing  from  the  muscle  spindle  while  thirty 
minutes  is  enough  to  abolish  the  initial  spike  (58). 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


137 


Thirty  minutes  soaking  is  about  the  time  needed 
to  abolish  the  impulse  from  a  Pacinian  corpuscle 
preparation  (37).  In  neither  instance  is  the  receptor 
potential  effected. 

The  times  of  action  of  these  solutions  are  remark- 
ably similar  for  the  two  preparations  and  in  both 
instances  are  very  long  compared  with  the  time  such 
solutions  take  to  act  on  isolated  single  nerve  fibers.  It 
seems  likely  that  diffusion  times  play  an  important 
part.  It  is  known  from  direct  experiments  with 
labelled  sodium,  potassium  and  bromine  that  diffu- 
sion through  the  capsules  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  is 
slow  C38). 

In  the  Pacinian  corpuscle,  however,  it  is  possible  to 
perfuse  the  receptor  through  the  capillary  loop  that 
enters  the  corpuscle  with  the  axon  and  ramifies  in  its 
proximal  pole  (19).  Using  a  perfused  preparation  of 
this  kind  it  is  found  that  procaine  in  a  concentration 
of  0.02  to  0.05  per  cent  in  the  perfusion  fluid  abolishes 
the  impulse;  0.05  per  cent  and  higher  concentrations 
of  procaine  cause  a  reduction  of  the  receptor  potential 
amplitude.  The  abolition  of  the  impulse  occurs  within 
1.5  min. 

If  these  preparations  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  are 
perfused  with  a  sodium-free  solution  the  amplitude  of 
the  receptor  potential  falls  and  after  about  20  min. 
the  amplitude  is  constant  and  very  small.  This  is 
illustrated  in  figure  9.  This  reduction  in  amplitude 
occurs  whether  the  sodium  chloride  of  the  physi- 
ological solution  is  replaced  by  choline  chloride  or 
by  sucrose.  The  effect,  under  faNoraisle  conditions, 
is  reversible  and  recovery  occurs  on  changing  the 
perfusion  fluid  back  to  a  physiological  solution. 
When  different  concentrations  of  sodium  are  per- 
fused it  is  found  that  the  amplitude  of  the  receptor 
potential,  measured  after  a  constant  le\el  has  been 
reached,  is  related  in  a  graded  manner  to  the  con- 
centration of  sodium.  When  sodium  is  absent  there 
is  a  small  remnant  of  the  receptor  potential;  it  is 
probable  that  this  represents  a  genuine  property 
of  the  receptor  (19). 

The  receptor  potentials  of  other  types  of  receptor 
have  also  been  found  to  be  resistant  to  local  anes- 
thetics. Cocaine  (0.5  per  cent)  applied  externally  has 
little  or  no  effect  on  the  potentials  of  the  olfactory 
mucous  memljrane,  though  the  same  application 
abolishes  the  responses  of  the  olfactory  bulb  (78). 
Procaine  in  concentrations  of  0.05  to  o.  i  per  cent  in 
the  bathing  fluid  abolishes  the  impulses  but  not  the 
receptor  potential  of  the  crayfish  stretch  receptor. 

The  position  at  the  present  time  seems  to  be  that 
while    receptor    potentials    are    more    resistant    than 


FIG.  9.  Effect  of  perfusion  with  a  sodium-free  solution  on 
receptor  potential  amplitude.  Abscissa:  time  in  min.  Ordninle: 
receptor  potential  amplitude,  arbitrary  units.  Sodium  chloride 
was  replaced  with  sucrose  and  changes  in  recording  resistance 
have  been  corrected  for.  Impulses  were  abolished  with  pro- 
caine but  were  allowed  to  reappear  during  the  period  marked 
by  the  dotted  line.  [From  Diamond,  J.,  J.  A.  B.  Gray  &  D. 
Inman.   Unpublished  figure.] 


impulses  to  procaine,  in  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  at  least 
quite  low  concentrations  (0.05  per  cent)  do  affect 
the  receptor  potential  if  the  diffusion  barriers  are 
avoided  by  perfusion.  Perfusion  also  reveals  that  the 
receptor  potential  is  almost  completely  abolished  in 
the  absence  of  sodium. 


TRANSMISSION  OF  ENERGY  TO  THE  RECEPTOR  ELEMENTS 

It  has  long  been  recognized  that  there  are  factors 
in  the  transmission  of  the  exciting  energy  to  the  re- 
ceptors that  are  important  in  the  functioning  of  the 


138 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


more  specialized  sense  organs.  For  example  the 
ability  of  the  cochlea  of  the  higher  vertebrates  to  act 
as  a  frequency  analyzer  is  due  to  its  mechanical  prop- 
erties (97).  In  compound  eyes  the  distribution  of 
absorbing  pigments  affects  the  distribution  of  light  on 
the  receptors  so  as  to  increase  cither  the  sensitivity 
or  the  discrimination  of  the  eye  (84).  The  same  situa- 
tion can  be  seen  if  the  skin  is  taken  as  a  whole.  It  has 
been  shown  that  thermal  receptors  respond  to  the 
temperature  at  a  given  point  at  a  given  time  (106); 
the  distribution,  both  in  time  and  space,  of  tempera- 
ture in  the  skin,  and  consccjuently  the  nature  of 
sensation  aroused,  will  depend  on  the  physical  proper- 
ties of  the  whole  system.  Another,  and  rather  different, 
example  of  the  effect  of  external  physical  factors  is 
the  decrease  in  the  rate  of  adaptation  of  mechanical 
receptors  in  frog's  skin  that  occurs  as  a  result  of 
stretching  the  skin  (68). 

All  the  examples  mentioned  in  the  last  paragraph 
refer  to  the  physical  properties  of  a  whole  tissue  or 
organ  and  their  effect  on  the  behavior  of  a  population 
of  receptors.  The  factors  involved  in  the  transmission 


of  energy  inside  what  is  normally  described  as  a 
single  ending  can  also  be  of  fundamental  importance. 
The  Pacinian  corpuscle  consists  of  a  central  core  sur- 
rounded by  thin  laminae  which  form  the  boundaries 
of  coaxial  spheroids;  the  spaces  between  the  laminae 
are  filled  with  fluid.  When  the  ending  is  .squeezed 
displacements  of  the  laminae  occur  and  these  can  be 
recorded  from  photographs  taken  with  short  flashes 
(51,  52).  During  and  immediately  after  the  onset  of  a 
compression,  relatively  large  displacements  of  the 
laminae  occur  (fig.  10  lejt);  but  these  decline  rapidly 
to  a  steady  value  which  is  maintained  as  long  as  the 
corpuscle  is  compressed.  This  maintained  displace- 
ment \aries  with  the  position  of  the  lamina  measured, 
those  near  the  periphery  of  the  corpuscle  showing  large 
displacements  while  those  near  the  center  show  none; 
figure  1 1  is  a  plot  of  maintained  displacement  against 
distance  from  the  center  of  the  corpuscle.  The  time 
course  of  the  compression  can  be  recorded  and  there- 
fore the  displacement  that  would  be  expected  at  any 
instant,  if  the  response  of  the  system  were  inde- 
pendent of  time,  can  be  calculated.  Subtraction  o 


0  2  4 


70 


so 


Fig.  II 


.III 


flOO 


II   200 


2>L 


300 


500 


600       M  700 


FIG.  10.  Mechanical  properties  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle.  Left:  time  course  of  displacements 
of  3  laminae  (see  inset)  during  a  compression  that  started  at  /  =  o,  rose  linearly  to  /  =  2.6  msec, 
and  then  remained  constant.  Right:  dynamic  component'  of  displacement.  See  text.  [By  courtesy 
of  S.J.  Hubbard.] 

FIG.  II.  Mechanical  properties  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle.  Abscissa:  diameter  in  the  transverse 
plane  (2r).  Ordinate:  maintained  displacement  of  laminae  as  functions  of  transverse  diameters 
(■2Ar).  t  marks  edge  of  the  central  core.  Bars  indicate  ±2  X  standard  error.  [By  courtesy  of  S 
J.  Hubbard.] 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT   RECEPTORS 


'39 


this  theoretical  displacement  from  that  observed 
(fig.  lo  left)  leaves  a  'dynamic  component'  (fig. 
10  right);  it  can  be  seen  that  this  component  is 
transmitted  with  less  attenuation  to  the  center  of  the 
end  organ,  and  also  that  its  time  course  is  similar  to 
that  of  a  receptor  potential  (fig.  4C).  It  seems  there- 
fore that  the  rapid  adaptation  of  this  receptor  is 
primarily  a  mechanical  phenomenon.  Since  neither  a 
change  in  axon  length  nor  a  bending  of  the  axon  has 
been  detected,  it  seems  that  radial  displacements  of 
the  axon  itself,  or  of  the  tissues  immediately  sur- 
rounding it,  are  responsible  for  activating  the  re- 
ceptor. 


EFFECTS  OF    TR.'SiNSMITTER     SUBSTANCES 

A  number  of  investigations  into  the  actions  of 
acetylcholine,  epinephrine,  histamine  and  related 
compounds  have  been  carried  out.  These  investiga- 
tions have  in  general  had  one  of  two  objecti\es:  one, 
to  see  if  these  substances  are  normally  involved  in  the 
initiation  of  impulses  by  receptors;  the  other,  to  see 
if  there  is  specialization  of  the  membrane  of  the  ter- 
minal part  of  the  sensory  axon. 

Action  of  Acetylcholine 

Acetylcholine  has  been  shown  to  increase  or  initiate 
a  discharge  of  impulses  from  a  variety  of  sensory 
receptors.  These  include  mechanical  receptors  from 
the  skin  of  the  cat  and  the  dog  (13,  23),  from  the  cat's 
carotid  sinus  (17,  64),  from  the  crayfish  stretch  re- 
ceptor (102),  the  cat's  tongue  (62)  and  from  the 
frog's  skin  (53);  also  thermal  receptors  in  the  cat's 
tongue  (21)  and  chemical  receptors  of  the  cat's 
tongue  (62)  and  carotid  body  (98).  Succinylcholine 
has  been  found  to  increase  the  activity  of  mammalian 
muscle  spindles  (33).  Finally  acetylcholine  has  been 
found  to  effect  and  even  initiate  sensations  in  the 
human  subject;  these  include  pain  (7,  44,  90)  and 
thermal  (10)  sensations.  Many  of  these  investigations 
include  control  experiments  designed  to  show  that 
these  are  direct  effects  on  the  sensory  pathway  and 
are  not  secondary  to  contractions  of  smooth  or  striated 
muscle  and  do  not  result  from  excitation  of  the  auto- 
nomic nervous  system.  It  seems  clear  therefore  that 
acetylcholine  does  have  an  action  on  some  part  of  the 
sensory  pathway,  and  since  similar  applications  of 
acetylcholine  to  nerve  fibers  (53,  70)  or  to  pregangli- 
onic nerve  terminals  (11,  14)  are  ineffective,  it  seems 
likely  that  these  results  represent  a  direct  action  of 


the  substance  on  the  receptor  mechanism  itself.  The 
dosage  and  pharmacological  pattern  of  these  re- 
sponses vary  from  one  preparation  to  another.  The 
most  common  picture  is  that  represented  by  the  ex- 
periments on  the  mechanical  receptors  of  cats  and 
frogs  in  which  responses  were  recorded  directly  from 
the  primary  sensory  nerve  fibers.  These  responses  are 
produced  by  doses  of  the  same  order  of  magnitude  as 
those  required  to  excite  the  skeletal  neuromuscular 
junction.  They  are  unaffected  by  atropine,  but  are 
blocked  by  curare  or  excess  nicotine;  smaller  doses 
of  nicotine  beha\e  like  acetylcholine.  The  picture  is 
thus  very  similar  to  that  of  the  acetylcholine  action  at 
synapses  and  the  skeletal  neuromuscular  junction. 
The  main  divergence  from  this  pattern  is  that  atropine 
blocks  the  acetylcholine  effect  in  the  crayfish  stretch 
receptor  (102).  Atropine  has  also  been  found  to  raise 
the  thresholds  for  the  sensations  of  pain  (90)  and  of 
cold  (lo)  in  the  human;  its  mode  of  action  in  these 
instances  is  not  at  present  clear. 

There  has  Ijeen  some  difference  of  opinion  as  to 
whether  acetylcholine  can  act  independently  or 
whether  it  merely  sensitizes  the  receptor  to  the 
natural  stimulus;  it  is  possible  that  the  action  may  be 
different  in  different  preparations.  In  some  prepara- 
tions, as  shown  in  figure  12,  there  is  no  doubt  that 
acetylcholine  can  initiate  a  discharge  (17)  and  that 
the  action  of  acetylcholine  summates  with  the  physio- 
logical stimulus  (17,  53).  In  the  frog's  skin  acetyl- 
choline does  not  effect  the  time  course  of  excitation 
or  recovery  but  does  lower  the  threshold  and  increase 
the  rate  of  adaptation  (53).  The  most  likely  explana- 
tion of  the  action  of  this  substance  is  that  it  depolarizes 
the  membrane  of  the  terminal  portions  of  the  sensory 
nerve  fiber  and  that  this  action  is  confined  to  those 
parts  that  take  part  in  the  generation  of  the  receptor 
potential.  This  conclusion  might  lead  one  to  suppose 
that  acetylcholine  plays  some  part  in  the  normal  re- 
sponse to  a  physiological  stimulus.  This,  however, 
seems  very  doul)tful  in  the  light  of  results  obtained 
with  blocking  agents  and  anticholinesterases. 

Action  oj  Blocking  Agents  and  Anticholinesterases 

It  has  been  stated  above  that  the  action  of  acetyl- 
choline on  sensory  receptors  is  blocked  by  curare.  It 
is  also  blocked  by  hexamethonium  (17,  23),  and  large 
doses  of  nicotine  (13).  While  these  substances  block 
the  action  of  a  subsequent  dose  of  acetylcholine  or 
nicotine,  they  have  no  effect,  in  most  preparations,  on 
the  normal  response  to  a  physiological  stimulus.  Thus 
the  mechanical  receptors  of  the  carotid  sinus  of  the 


140 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^-^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


iliiiiM* 


mmi 


\mmmmmmmmi\m\mmmi\m\M 


FIG.  12.  Acetylcholine  excitation  of  pressure  receptors  in  the  cat's  carotid  sinus,  a:  pressure  in 
sinus,  25  mm  Hg;  injection  of  0.5  ml  saline,  b:  same  pressure;  injection  of  0.5  ml  of  io~'  g  per  ml 
acetylcholine,  c:  pressure,  iii  mm  Hg;  i.o  ml  saline,  d:  same  pressure;  1.0  ml  acetylcholine  10  » 
g  per  ml.  Time,  o.  i  sec.  All  records  made  95  sec.  after  injection.  [From  Diamond  (17).] 


cat  are  still  able  to  produce  a  normal  frequency- 
pressure  curve  when  perfused  with  i  per  cent  hexa- 
methonium  (fig.  13),  although  the  acetylcholine  effect 
is  lilocked  lay  a  concentration  of  io~^  he.xamethonium 
(17).  In  the  case  of  the  carotid  body,  the  chemical 
receptors  of  which  appear  particularly  sensitive  to 
acetylcholine,  large  doses  of  blocking  agents  diminisn 
the  response  to  low  oxygen  tensions  (61). 

Physostigmine  does  not  affect  the  response  of 
mechanical  receptors  in  cat's  and  dog's  skin  to  me- 
chanical stimulation  (13),  nor  does  it  alter  the 
pressure-frequency  relationship  of  the  pressure  re- 
ceptors of  the  cat's  carotid  sinus  (17).  In  two  types  of 
chemical  receptor,  anticholinesterases  do  enhance  the 
response  to  the  physiological  stimulus;  thus  physo- 
stigmine and  prostigmine  increase  the  activity  of 
chemical  receptors  of  the  cat's  carotid  sinus  and 
prostigmine  increases  that  of  chemical  receptors  in 
the  cat's  tongue. 

These  results  suggest  that  acetylcholine  cannot  be 
an  intermediary  in  the  normal  process  of  excitation 
at  many  types  of  receptor.  Against  this  evidence,  it 
has  been  argued  that  the  blocking  agents  do  not  have 
access  to  the  critical  region;  however,  all  these  agents 
block  the  acetylcholine  effect  and  nicotine  is  both  an 
exciting  and  blocking  agent.  Such  arguments  can  only 
be  valid  if  it  is  argued  that  there  is  a  third  region  on 
the  sensory  pathway  that  differs  from  the  main  part 
of  the  neuron  in  its  sensitivity  to  these  substances  and 
from  the  receptor  region  in  that  it  is  not  involved  in 
the  production  of  receptor  potentials.  There  is  no 
evidence  that  acetylcholine  is  present  in  receptors 
(13),  but  there  is  evidence  of  the  presence  of  cholin- 


esterase  in  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  (8,  43)  and  Meiss- 
ner's  corpuscle  (8);  in  the  former  this  appears  to  be  all 
pseudocholinesterase  and  its  destruction  does  not 
appear  to  effect  function  in  any  way  during  an  acute 
experiment  (Diamond,  J.  &  J.  A.  B.  Gray,  unpub- 
lished obser\ationsJi. 

The  arguments  against  the  participation  of  acetyl- 
choline as  an  intermediary  in  the  normal  process  of 
excitation  of  some  types  of  receptor  do  not  exclude 
the  possibility  that  local  concentrations  of  acetyl- 
choline may  modify  the  excitability  of  receptors  under 
physiological  conditions.  There  is  no  evidence  for 
such  an  action  of  acetylcholine,  but  there  is  evidence 
that  a  parallel  action  can  occur  with  epinephrine. 

Effects  0/  Sympathetic  Stimulation  and  Epmep/inne 

Stimulation  of  the  sympathetic  supply  to  the  skin  of 
the  frog  has  been  shown  to  increase  the  excitability  of 
the  cutaneous  receptors  (67).  Stimulation  of  the  sym- 
pathetic in  these  preparations  increases  the  response 
to  a  standard  mechanical  stimulus  applied  to  the  skin 
surface;  also  if  the  skin  is  stretched  biu  not  otherwise 
stimulated  mechanically  so  that  there  is  no  discharge 
in  the  aflferent  fibers,  stimulation  of  the  sympathetic 
may  initiate  a  discharge.  These  results  are  paralleled 
by  the  application  of  epinephrine  to  the  skin.  The 
effects  of  epinephrine  and  sympathetic  stimulation 
add  to  those  of  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  skin 
and  the  application  of  currents  to  it.  These  results  have 
been  obtained  in  preparations  which  have  been  sub- 
.sequently  sectioned  and  shown  to  contain  no  smooth 
muscle  except  that  associated  with  the  blood  vessels 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


141 


40 


30  - 


X 
o      • 


20 


10 


_  £ 


•o 

X 


60 


-•-i- 


100 


-o-»J 


J_ 


_L 


_L 


220 


260 


140  180 

Pressure,  mm  Hg 

FIG.  13.  Effects  of  hexamethonium  on  the  pressure-response 
relationship  of  cat's  carotid  sinus  receptors.  Abscissa/  Pressure 
in  sinus  in  mm  Hg.  Ordinate:  impulse  frequency  per  sec.  O, 
X  normal  curves,  •  perfusion  with  i  per  cent  hexamethonium. 
[From  Diamond  (17).] 


and  they  appear  to  be  due  to  a  direct  effect  of  epi- 
nephrine on  the  receptor.  Epinephrine  can  also  in- 
crease the  .size  of  the  receptor  potential  of  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  in  response  to  a  given  stimulus;  this  results 
in  a  lowering  of  the  threshold  (6g).  In  the  carotid 
sinus  of  the  cat  there  is  also  an  effect  of  epinephrine, 
but  in  this  instance  the  effect  appears  to  be  secondary 
to  its  action  on  the  muscle  of  the  sinus  (17,  63). 

These  results  show  that  the  activity  of  receptors 
may  be  modified  by  centrifugal  activity.  The  idea 
is  not,  of  course,  new  because  the  effects  of  stimulating 
the  efferent  fibers  to  the  muscle  spindles  are  well 
known  (59).  Centrifugal  influences  on  the  activity  of 
the  ear  (29)  and  eye  (31)  are  also  under  investigation, 
but  whether  or  not  these  operate  at  receptor  level  is 
not  yet  clear.  This  topic  is  discussed  also  by  Livingston 
(Chapter  XXXI)  on  central  effects  on  afferent  activ- 
ity in  this  work. 

Othn  Substances 

Histamine  is  a  substance  that  has  been  much  inves- 
tigated in  relation  to  receptors,  especially  those  con- 


cerned with  the  sensation  of  pain  in  man.  Discussion 
of  this  problem  belongs  to  another  chapter.  Many 
other  agents  have  also  been  investigated  (80)  and 
special  mention  should  be  made  of  the  sensitization  of 
receptors  by  anesthetics  (88,  loi). 


MINUTE  STRUCTURE  OF  RECEPTORS 

Electronmicroscopical  studies  have  begun  to  throw 
some  light  on  those  structural  relationships  that  mav 
be  of  importance  in  explaining  the  genesis  of  the  re- 
ceptor potential  in  mechanical  receptors.  Sections  of 
muscle  spindles  from  the  frog  and  of  Pacinian  cor- 
puscles from  the  cat's  mesentery  have  been  investi- 
gated. 

In  the  muscle  spindle  the  finer  branches  of  the  af- 
ferent filler  which  are  nonmedullated  lie  in  close  rela- 
tion to  the  intrafusal  muscle  fiber.  These  fibers,  as  they 
approach  their  termination,  lose  their  Schwann  cell 
sheath  and  come  into  direct  contact  with  the  muscle 
fibers;  the  continuation  of  the  Schwann  cell  also  runs 
in  contact  with  the  muscle  but  is  separated  from  the 
axon.  Smaller  axons,  which  may  represent  the  final 
terminations,  are  also  seen  in  close  relation  to,  but 
not  in  contact  with,  the  muscle  surface.  The  terminal 
parts  of  the  afferent  fibers  contain  many  mito- 
chondria, though  with  no  apparent  orientation 
(fig.  14.4)  (87). 

In  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  the  axon  is  nonmyeli- 
nated from  the  point  at  which  it  enters  the  central 
core  (86).  At  this  point  it  has  a  diameter  of  2  /x  which 
it  maintains  until  it  ends.  Over  the  whole  of  this 
nonmyelinated  section  there  are  certain  characteristic 
features  (85)  (fig.  145).  There  appears  to  be  no 
Schwann  cell  sheath;  there  are  numerous  mitochon- 
dria inside  the  nerve  fiber  arranged  as  a  palisade 
around  the  fiber  just  beneath  its  surface  membrane. 
The  axon  itself  is  not  round  but  an  ellipse  in  cross 
section  and  is  surrounded  by  a  complex  cellular 
structure.  This  cellular  structure  is  divided  into  two 
D-shaped  parts  separated  from  each  other,  in  the 
middle  by  the  axon,  and  on  either  side  by  gaps  that 
continue  the  plane  of  the  long  axis  of  the  elliptical 
nerve  fiber. 

At  this  stage  of  such  investigations,  the  most  striking 
feature  of  the.se  results  is  that  both  types  of  mechanical 
receptors  show  the  terminal  axon  without  a  Schwann 
cell  sheath. 


142 


HANDBOOK    OF    l'H%SIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


inner  sh 


FIG.  14A.  Diagram  of  a  cross  section  of  a  portion  of  a  frog's 
muscle  spindle,  at  resting  length,  in  the  region  of  the  sensory 
innervation.  Inner  sh.,  intrafusal  muscle  fiber  inner  sheath; 
m.nuc,  muscle  nuclei;  mf.,  myofilaments;  sarc,  sarcoplasm; 
m.,  mitochondria;  peri,  subst.,  perimuscular  substance;  ax., 
axons;  Sch.,  Schwann  cells.  [By  courtesy  of  J.  D.  Robertson.] 


FIG.  14B.  Diagram  of  a  transverse  section  of  the  central  core 
of  a  Pacinian  corpuscle  based  on  electronmicrographs.  [By 
courtesy  of  A.  Quilliam.] 


HYPOTHESES    CONCERNING    THE    MECHANISMS 
OF    RECEPTORS 

Many  of  our  present  ideas  on  the  mechanisms  in- 
volved in  the  initiation  of  impulses  by  receptors  stem 
from  the  idea  of  nerve  as  a  model  sense  organ  (9). 
This  concept  invokes  two  parts;  first  that  a  constant 
current  would  excite  repetitive  discharges  in  a  nerve 
fiiaer,  and  secondly  that  such  currents  are  produced 
in  nerves  under  physiological  conditions  by  the  devel- 
opment of  generator  potentials  in  the  receptors. 

It  is  now  known  that  many  receptors  produce  recep- 
tor potentials  and  it  is  probably  safe  to  assume  that 
this  is  a  generalization  that  applies  widely.  There  is 
good  evidence,  which  has  already  been  considered, 
that  these  receptor  potentials  are  the  immediate  cause 
of  the  impulse  discharges.  At  present  there  is  no  evi- 
dence or  need  to  suppose  that  the  part  of  the  afferent 
fiber  in  which  the  impulses  are  .set  up  differs  from 
other  parts  of  nerve  fibers  in  its  response  to  a  flow  of 
current,  whether  this  be  a  flow  of  current  due  to  a 
receptor  potential,  to  an  external  source  or  to  the 
summated  effects  of  both.  There  is  evidence  that  has 
already  been  considered  which  indicates  that  in  the 
Pacinian  corpuscle  impulses  are  set  up  at  the  first 
node  of  Ransier  and  that  the  terminal  nonmyelinated 
portion  of  the  nerve  fiber  does  not  appear  capable  of 
conducting  impulses.  Similar  conclusions  can  be 
drawn  for  the  stretch  receptor  of  the  crayfish,  though 
in  this  instance  it  is  not  possible  to  put  such  clear  ana- 
tomical limits  to  impulse  conduction.  It  may  well  be 
a  general  property  of  receptors  that  impulses  are  set  up 
at  a  point  central  to  the  sensitive  terminals  by  currents 
which  are  generated  elsewhere.  The  summation  noted 
between  natural  stimuli  and  externally  applied  cur- 
rents would  result  from  a  passive  summation  of  the 
discharging  process  in  this  region  of  the  membrane. 

The  results  just  considered  further  suggest  that  the 
part  of  the  nerve  fiber  that  is  unable  to  conduct  a  nerve 
impulse  is  the  site  at  which  the  receptor  potential  is 
generated.  This  view  is  supported  by  the  fact  that  the 
conditions  under  w  hich  receptor  potentials  have  been 
recorded  from  the  three  mechanical  receptors  indi- 
cate that  the  current  must  have  crossed  the  nerse  fiber 
membrane  peripheral  to  the  point  of  recording.  Esti- 
mates of  the  absolute  value  of  the  maximum  receptor 
potentials  suggest  that  it  is  unlikely  that  those  currents 
that  traverse  the  membrane  of  the  nerve  terminal  are 
secondary  to  acti\ity  in  an  external  source.  Further- 
more the  fine  structure  of  these  terminals  shows  certain 
distinctive  features.  Thus,  to  take  the  specific  example 
of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle,  the  nonmyelinated  terminal 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


143 


appears  unable  to  conduct  impulses  and  this  same 
region  is  structurally  specialized,  in  particular  in  not 
having  a  Schwann  cell  sheath.  The  estimated  poten- 
tial change  that  occurs  across  the  membrane  of  this 
part  of  the  fiber  during  a  maximum  receptor  potential 
is  of  the  same  order  of  magnitude  as  the  resting  and 
action  potentials.  Many  receptors  are  sensitive  to 
acetylcholine,  though  it  is  not  known  whether  or  not 
the  Pacinian  corpuscle  is  sensitive.  It  is  tempting  to 
suggest  that  the  inability  to  conduct  impulses,  the 
sensitivity  to  acetylcholine,  the  ability  to  produce  re- 
ceptor potentials  and  the  absence  of  the  Schwann  cell 
sheath  are  all  connected.  There  is  not  howe\er  enough 
evidence  at  present  to  support  such  an  assertion. 

Olfactory  receptors  appear  to  fall  in  line  with  much 
of  what  has  been  said  in  the  last  few  paragraphs.  To 
some  extent  photoreceptors  may  as  well,  but  these 
considerations  belong  to  other  chapters.  For  the  rest 
of  this  discussion  consideration  will  be  given  almost 
entirely  to  simple  mechanical  receptors  as  it  is  from 
receptors  of  this  type  that  the  relevant  evidence  is  at 
present  available. 

The  next  point  to  be  considered  is  the  immediate 
source  of  energy  utilized  in  the  production  of  a  recep- 
tor potential.  Maintained  receptor  potentials  that  last 
for  minutes  have  been  recorded,  and  if  it  is  assumed 
that  receptor  potentials  are  responsible  for  the  initia- 
tion of  impulses  in  certain  other  receptors,  for  example 
the  mechanical  receptors  in  the  carotid  sinus  of  the 
cat,  receptor  potentials  must  remain  constant  for 
hours  (17).  Such  potentials  cannot  be  maintained 
across  a  biological  membrane  without  the  continual 
utilization  of  energy;  such  energy  clearly  cannot  be 
provided  by  the  work  done  during  the  deformation  of 
the  receptor.  Since  this  is  so,  an  internal  store  of 
energy  must  be  available.  It  has  already  been  argued 
that  receptor  potentials  are  generated  across  the  mem- 
brane of  the  terminal  portions  of  the  afferent  nerve 
fiber.  Across  this  membrane  is  a  store  of  energy  in  the 
form  of  the  electrochemical  gradients  of  the  principal 
ions.  It  seems  likely  that  it  is  this  energy  which  is 
utilized  during  the  activity  of  the  receptor.  In  all 
slowly  adapting  receptors  some  such  internal  store  of 
energy  must  be  available;  this  does  not  necessarily 
follow  for  rapidly  adapting  processes  such  as  the  re- 
ceptor potential  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle  and  the 
dynamic  phase  of  the  muscle  spindle  potential.  While 
it  seems  reasonable  that  all  mechanical  receptors  of 
the  relatively  simple  group  under  consideration  should 
have  fundamentally  the  same  mechanism,  there  is  no 
conclusive  evidence  that  this  is  so.  In  fact  it  has  been 
suggested  (58)  that  the  static  and  dynamic  phases  of 


the  muscle  spindle  receptor  potential  may  have  dif- 
ferent mechanisms. 

During  receptor  activity  the  potential  across  this 
membrane  must  alter.  One  suggestion  discussed  as  an 
explanation  of  the  dynamic  phase  of  the  muscle 
spindle  receptor  potential  was  that  the  potential 
change  was  a  result  of  a  change  of  membrane  capacity, 
the  total  charge  remaining  constant;  it  was,  however, 
pointed  out  that  there  were  quantitative  difficulties  in 
this  explanation  (58).  Similar  calculations  for  the 
Pacinian  corpuscle  demand  large  increases  in  surface 
area  which  are  known  not  to  occur.  A  inore  likely 
explanation  of  receptor  activity  is  that  ions  transfer 
charge  across  the  membrane  by  moving  down  their 
electrochemical  gradients  as  a  result  of  changes  in  the 
permeability  of  the  membrane  to  one  or  more  ion 
species  (37,  58).  If  charge  is  to  be  transferred  in  such  a 
direction  as  to  explain  the  observed  potential  changes, 
cations  must  enter  the  fiber  or  anions  leave  it.  The 
internal  anions  of  nerve  fibers  are  mostly  large  and 
less  likely  to  move  than  the  external  cations  which  are 
almost  entirely  .sodium.  If  the  mechanism  in  question 
were  something  of  the  kind  suggested,  it  would  then  be 
expected  that  the  receptor  potential  would  be  nearly 
abolished  in  the  absence  of  sodium.  This  is  in  fact 
what  has  been  observed  in  the  Pacinian  corpuscle. 
Another  observation  that  can  be  explained  on  this 
hypothesis  is  that  the  rate  of  rise  of  the  receptor  poten- 
tial continues  to  increase  with  increasing  stimulus 
strength  at  a  level  of  stimulus  strength  at  which  the 
amplitude  of  the  potential  change  remains  practically 
constant.  This  can  be  explained  by  assuming  that  the 
permeability  of  the  membrane  continues  to  increase, 
so  increasing  the  rate  at  which  charge  is  transferred, 
while  the  final  potential  reached  is  limited  by  the 
equiliijrium  potentials  of  the  ionic  gradients  con- 
cerned. 

If  the  hypothesis  put  forward  be  accepted  for  the 
time  being,  the  next  problem  is  to  consider  how  the 
changes  of  membrane  permeability  are  brought  about. 
This  might  be  due  to  a  distortion  of  the  membrane  or 
displacements  in  relation  to  surrounding  structures, 
it  might  be  due  to  a  change  of  pressure  in  and  around 
the  axon  or  there  may  be  chemical  intermediaries 
outside  or  inside  the  axon.  The  last  alternative  still 
leaves  the  problem  of  how  the  mechanical  energy 
produces  the  chemical  intermediaries.  At  present  there 
are  no  grounds  for  choosing  between  these  mecha- 
nisms. However,  if  there  are  chemical  intermediaries  in 
the  Pacinian  corpuscle,  the  time  course  of  their  action 
(the  latency  often  being  less  than  0.2  msec.)  and  their 
ability  to  function  at  room  temperature  (37)  show 


'44 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


that  they  have  quite  different  properties  from  main- 
iiialian  synaptic  transmitters.  Other  e\idence  has  been 
presented  in  an  earlier  section  which  suggests  that  at 
many  mechanical  receptors  acetylcholine  does  not  act 
as  a  chemical  transmitter. 

The  adaptation  of  receptors  is  a  subject  that  has 
stimulated  many  hypotheses  (47,  74).  Until  recently 
these  have  been  based  mostly  on  the  concept  that 
the  adaptation  of  receptors  is  closely  related  to  the 
accommodation  of  nerve  fibers.  It  would  certainly 
be  expected  that  this  factor  would  play  a  part  if 
impulses  are  set  up  in  the  nerve  fiber  as  a  result  of 
currents  generated  by  receptor  activity  in  the  ter- 
minals. This  factor  cannot  be  entirely  discounted,  but 
there  are  now  very  good  reasons  for  supposing  that 
other  factors  may  be  more  important.  The  time 
courses  of  all  the  receptor  potentials  so  far  observed 
are  in  general  agreement  with  the  corresponding 
time  courses  of  the  impulse  discharge.  Thus  the 
short  receptor  potential  of  the  Pacinian  corpuscle 
corresponds  to  the  single  impulse  produced  by  rela- 
tively large  stimuli,  the  dynamic  and  static  phases  of 
the  receptor  potential  of  the  muscle  spindle  corre- 
spond to  the  initial  high  frequency  burst  and  the 
maintained  discharge  of  impulses  and  the  receptor 
potentials  found  in  the  two  types  of  stretch  receptor 
investigated  in  the  crayfish  correspond  to  the  fast 
and  slow  adaptation  of  the  two  endings.  The  adapta- 
tion of  the  receptor  potential  inay  simply  reflect 
changes  in   the  mechanical   events  going  on  in   the 


terminals.  This  seems  to  ije  the  case  in  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  where  only  a  brief  wave  of  distortion  can 
be  found  in  the  central  core  during  a  maintained 
deformation  of  the  outside  of  the  endorgan.  In  the 
crustacean  stretch  receptors,  the  difference  between 
the  slow  and  fast  cells  has  been  attributed  to  differ- 
ences in  the  mechanical  attachments  between  the 
dendrites  of  the  two  types  of  cell  and  the  muscle 
fibers  in  which  they  ramify  (27).  The  change  in  the 
rate  of  adaptation  of  receptors  in  frog  skin,  when  the 
skin  is  stretched,  is  another  example  of  the  importance 
of  mechanical  factors.  It  is  impossible  to  say  whether 
or  not  such  factors  can  account  for  the  whole  phe- 
nomenon of  adaptation  of  the  receptor  potential.  It 
is  possible  that  there  may  be  some  mechanism  that 
reduces  the  effectiveness  of  a  stimulus  as  time  passes; 
such  a  mechanism  might  conceivably  be  related  to 
the  depression  of  the  receptor  potential  observed  in 
the  Pacinian  corpuscle. 

Many  of  our  ideas  on  the  mechanisms  of  receptors 
are  at  the  present  time  speculative.  Definite  ideas  on 
these  problems  may  develop  as  work  goes  deeper 
into  the  mechanisms  of  those  few  receptors  which  are 
particularly  well  adapted  for  such  investigations.  Also 
when  results,  that  ha\e  already  been  obtained  on 
some  receptors,  are  repeated  or  contradicted  by 
work  on  other  types,  it  may  be  possible  to  say  how- 
far  we  may  generalize  from  such  results  as  have  been 
obtained. 


REFERENCES 

I.   Adrian,   E.   D.    The  Basis  oj  Sensation.   London:   Christo- 
phers, 1928. 
■2.   .\dri.\n,  E.  D.  Pr9c.  Roy.  ,S'o<-.,  London,  ser.  B.   log:  i,  1931. 

3.  ■•\drian,   E.   D.,   McK.   Catell  and   H.   Ho.agland.  J. 
Physiol.  72:  377,  1931. 

4.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  J.  P/iyuol.  61:   151, 
1926. 

5.  Adrian,  E.   D.   and  Y.  Zotterm.an.  J.  Physio/.  61 :  465, 
1926. 

6.  Alvarez-Buvll.'\,  R.  .^^nd  J.  Ramirez  de  .\rellano.  Am. 
J.  Physiol.  172:  237,  1953. 

7.  Armstrong,  D.,  R.   M.   L.   Dry,   C.   .\.   Keele  and  J. 
W.  Markham.  J.  Physiol.  120:  236,  1953. 

8.  Beckett,  Evelyn  B.,  G.  H.  Bourne  and  \V.  Mont.agn.\. 
J.  Physiol.  134:  202,  1956. 

9.  Bernhard,  C.  G.,  R.  Granit  and  C.  R.  Skoglund.  J. 
Neurophysiol.  5:  55,  1942. 

10.  Bing,  H.  I.  and  a.  p.  Skoubv.  Ada  physiol.  scaiidinav.  21: 
286,  1950. 

11.  Brink,  F.,  D.  VV.  Bronk  and  M.  Larrabee.  Ann.  New 
Tork  Acad.  Sc.  47 :  457,   1 946. 

12.  Bronk,  D.  VV.  and  G.  Stella.  Am.  J.  Physiol,   no:  708, 

'935- 

13.  Brown,  G.   L.  and  J.  .\.  B.  Gray.  J.  Phynol.   107:  306, 

1948. 


14.  Brovvn,   G.    L.   and   F.   C.   Macintosh.  J.   Ptnsiol.   g6' 
loP,  1939. 

15.  Bullock,   T.   H.   and   F.   P.  J.   Diecke.  J.   Physiol.    134 

47.  '95<^- 

16.  D.AVis,    H.,    I.    T.-^s.^Ki    and    R.    Goldstein.    Cold   Spring 
Harbor  Symp.   17:   143,    1952. 

17.  Diamond,  J.  J.  Physiol.  130:513,  1955- 

18.  Diamond,  J.,  J.  .A.  B.  Gray-  and  D.  R.  Inman.  J.  Phys- 
iol. 141:  117,  1958. 

19.  Diamond,  J.,  J.  .\.  B.  Gray  and  D.  R    Inman.  J.  Physiol. 
142:  382,  1958. 

20.  Diamond,  J.,  j.  .\.  B.  Gray  and  M.   Sato.  J.   Plnuol. 

133:  54.  1956- 

21.  DoDT,  E.,  .\.  p.  Skouby  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Ada  phyuol. 

scandinav.   28:    loi,    1953. 

22.  Dodt,  E.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26: 

345.  '95^- 

23.  Douglas,  VV.  \V.  and  J.  A.  B.  Gray.  J.   Physiol.    119: 

n8,  1953- 

24.  EcKER,   .-V.    AND    R.    VViedershein.    Anatomic  des   l-'rosches. 
Braunschweig:  Vieweg,  1896. 

25.  Edward,  C.  J.  Physiol.  127:  636,  1955. 

26.  Erlanger,  J.  AND  E.  .\.  Blair.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  121  :  431, 
■938. 


INITIATION    OF    IMPULSES    AT    RECEPTORS 


145 


27.  EVZAGUIRRE,     C.     AND     S.     \\  .     KuFFLER.     J.     Oe?l.     Physiol. 
39:  87,    1955- 

28.  EvzAGUiRRE,   C.    AND   S.   \V.    KuFFLER.   J.    Grn.    Physiol. 

39:    '21,    1955. 

29.  Galambos,    R.    XXth   Internal.    Phniol.    Cong.,    Abstracts   of 
Communications:  ^21,   1956. 

30.  Granit,    R.    Sensory    Mechanisms    of   the    Retina.    London: 
Oxford,  1947. 

31.  Granit,   R.    Receptors  and  .Semory    Perception.    New   Ha\'en : 
Vale,  1955. 

32.  Granit,    R.    and    C.    R.    Skoglund.   J.    .\eiirophyslot.    6: 

337.  '943- 

33.  Granit,  R.,  S.  Skoglund  and  S.  Thesleff.  Acta  physiol. 
scandlnav.   28:    134,    1953. 

34.  Gray,  J.    A.    B.    and  J.    L.    Malcolm.    Proc.    Rin.    Soc, 
London,  ser.  B  137:  96,   1950. 

35.  Gray,  J.  A.  B.  and  J.  L.  Malcolm.  J.  Physiol.    115:  i, 

1951- 

36.  Gray,  J.  A.  B.  and  P.  B.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  114: 

454.  19.51- 

37.  Gray,  J.  A.  B.  and  M.  Sato.  J.  Physiol.  122:  610,  1953. 

38.  Gray,  J.  A.  B.  and  M.  Sato.  J.  Physiol.  129:  594,  1955. 

39.  Hagen,  E.,  H.  Knocke,  D.  C.  Sinclair  and  G.  Weddell. 
Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  141  :  279,  1953. 

40.  Hartline,  H.  K.,  N.  a.  Coulter  and  H.  G.  Wagner 
Fed.  Proc.   II;  65,    1952. 

41.  Hartline,  H.  K.,  and  C.  H.  Graham.  J.  Cell  &  Comp. 
Physiol.   1  :  277,    1932. 

42.  Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  E.  F.  MacNichol 
Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.   17;  125,   1952. 

43.  Hebb,    Catherine    and    K.     J.    Hill,    (hiarl.    J.    Exper. 
Physiol.  40:   168,   1955. 

44.  Hellauer,  H.  p.  .^tschr.  reri;l.  Physiol.  32:  303,   1950. 

45.  Hensel,    H.    and    Y.    Zotterman.    J.    Physiol.     115:    16, 

'95"- 

46.  Hensel,   H.   and  Y.   Zotterm.\n.   Acta  physiol.   scandinav. 

23:  ^91.  '95>- 

47.  HoAGLAND,  H.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  4;  347,   1936. 

48.  HoDGKiN,   A.    L.    Proc.   Roy.   Soc,   London,   ser    B    126:  87, 
1938. 

49.  HoDGKiN,  A.  L.  J.  Physiol.  107:  165,  1948. 

50.  Hogg,  B.  M.  J.  Physiol.  84:  250,  1935. 

51.  Hubbard,  S.J.  J.  Physiol.  132:  23P,  1956. 

52.  Hubbard,  S.  J.  J.  Physiol.  141 :  198,  1958. 

53.  Jarrett,  a.  S.  J.  Physiol.  133:  243,  1956. 

54.  JiELOF,  R.,  A.  Spoor  and  H.  de  Vries.  J.  Physiol.   116: 

'37.  '952- 

55.  Katsuki,    Y.    and    S.    Yoshino.    Jap.   J.    Physiol.    2:    219, 

'95^- 

56.  Katz,  B.  J.  Phtsiol.  106:  66,  1947. 

57.  Katz,  B.  J.  Physiol.   11 1:  248,  1950. 

58.  Katz,  B.  J.  Physiol.  1 1 1  :  261,  1950. 

59.  KuFFLER,  S.  W.,  C.  C.  Hunt  and  J.   P.  Quilliam.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.   14:  29,   1951. 

60.  L.^NDGREN,  S.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26:  i,  1952. 

61.  Landgren,    S.,    G.    Liljestrand    and    Y.    Zotterman. 
Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26:  264,   1952. 

62.  Landgren,    S.,    G.    Liljestrand    and    Y.    Zotterman. 
Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  30:   105,   1954. 

63.  Landgren,  S.,  E.  Neil  and  Y.  Zotterman.  .icta  physiol. 
scandinav.  25:  24,  1952. 

64.  Landgren,  S.,  A.   P.   Skouby  .and  Y.   Zotterman.   Acta 
physiol.  scandinav.  29:  381,   1953. 


65- 
66. 

67. 
68. 
69. 


7'- 
7^- 

73- 

74- 
75- 
76. 


77- 
78. 

79- 
80. 


82. 
83- 

84. 


86. 
87. 

88. 


90. 
9'- 
9^- 

93- 
94- 
95- 
96- 
97- 


98. 

99- 
100. 


103. 
104. 
105. 
106. 


Lele,  p.  p.  J.  Physiol.  126:  191,  1954. 

Lele,  p.  p.,  G.  Weddell  and  C.  M.  Williams.  J.  Physiol. 

126:  206,  1954. 

Loewenstein,  W.  R.  J.  Physiol.  132:  40,  1956. 

Loewenstein,  W.  R.  J.  Physiol.  133:  588,  1956. 

Loewenstein,    W.    R.    and    R.    Altamirano-Orrego. 

.\ature,  London   178:   1292,    1956. 

Lorente  de  No,  R.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  24:  85,  1944. 

Lowenstein,  O.  J'.  Physiol.  127:  104,  1955. 

Lowenstein,  O.  and  A.  S.\nd.  Proc.  Roy.  .Soc,  London,  ser. 

B  129;  256,  1940. 

Maruhashi,  J.,  K.  MizuGUCHi  and  L  Tasaki.  J.  Physiol. 

"7:  1^9.  '95^- 

Matthews,  B.  H.  C.  J.  Physiol.  71  :  64,  1931. 

Matthews,  B.  H.  C.  J.  Physiol.  78:  i,  1933. 

MiJLLER,   J.    Handbuch   der    Physiologie    des   .Mensclien.    Co- 

blenz:  J.  Holscher,    1840.  (Parts  translated   by  \V.   Baly. 

The    Physiology   of  the   Senses,    Voice   and   Muscular    .Motion. 

London:  Taylor  Walton  &   Maberly,    1848.) 

Murray,  R.  W.  J.  Physiol.  134:  408,  1956. 

Ottoson,  D.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  35:  Suppl.   122,  1956. 

Ottoson,    D.    and    G.    Svaetichin.    Cold   Spring    Harbor 

Symp.    17:    165,    1952. 

Paintal,   a.   S.   X.Xth  Internal.   PhyMol.   Cong.,   Abstracts  oj 

Reviews:    78,    1956. 

Parrack,  H.  O.  ,4m.  J.  Physiol.   130:  481,   1940. 

Pfaffmann,  C.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  17:  243,  1941. 

PiERON,   H.    The  Sensations.    Their  Functions,   Processes  and 

Mechanisms.  London:  MuUer,  1952. 

Prosser,    C.    L.    (editor).    Comparative    .Animal    Physiology. 

Philadelphia:  Saunders,  1950. 

Quilliam,  T.  A.  Fed.  Proc.  15:  147,  1956. 

Quilliam,  T.  A.  and  M.  Sato.  J.  Physiol.  129:  167,  1955. 

Robertson,    J.    D.    Proc.    ist   European   Reg.  Conf.   Electron 

.Microsc:    197,    1957. 

Robertson,  J.  D.,  A.  A.  B.  Sw.\n  .and  D.  Whitteridge. 

J.   Physiol.    131;  463,   1956. 

Skoglund,    C.    R.    Acta  physiol.   scandinav.    4:   Suppl.    12, 

'94'^- 

Skouby,  A.   P.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  24:   174,   1951. 
Svaetichin,  G.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav. 'i^:  Suppl.  134,  1956. 
Tasaki,    L    .Nervous    Transmission.    Springfield:    Thomas, 

'953- 

Tasaki,  L  J.  .Neurophysiol.  17:  97,  1954. 
Tasaki,  L  and  M.  Sak.\guchi.  Jap.  J.  Physiol,  i  :  7,  1950. 
VAN  Leeuwen,  S.  J.  Physiol.  109:  142,  1949. 
von  Bekesy,  G.  J.  Acoust.  Soc.  Am.  24:  72,  1952. 
von  Bekesy,  G.  and  W.  A.  Rosenblith.   In:   The  Hand- 
book of  Experimental  Psychology,  edited   by   S.   S.   Ste\ens. 
New  York:  Wiley,   1951. 

von  Euler,  U.  S.,  G.  Liljestrand  and  Y.  Zotterman. 
Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  i :  383,  1 94 1  ■ 
Wedell,  G.  Ann.  Rev.  Psychol.  6:   119,   1955. 
Wever,    E.   G.    and   C.   W.   Bray.    Proc.   .Nat.    Acad.   ,SV., 
Wash.   16:  344,   1930. 

Whitteridge,    D.    and   E.    Bulbring.   J.    Pharmacol.    81. 
340,  1944. 

Wiersma,  C.   a.   G.,   E.   Furshpan  and  E.   Florey.  J 
Exper.  Biol.   30:    136,    1953. 

Zotterman,  Y.  Skandtnav.  .-irch.  Physiol.  75:  105,   1936 
Zotterman,  Y.  J.  Physiol.  95:  i,  1939- 
Zotterman,  Y.  J.  Physiol.  102:  313,  1943. 
Zotterman.  Y.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  15:  357,  1953. 


CHAPTER    V 


Synaptic  and  ephaptic  transmission 


HARRY    GRUNDFEST 


Department   of  Neurology,   College   of  Physicians   and  Surgeons, 
Columbia  University,  New  York  City 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Nature  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Generation  Sites  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Molecular  Structures  of  Differently  Excitable  Membranes 

Types  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Interrelations  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials  and  Spikes 
Specific  Properties  of  Synaptic  Electrogenesis 

Evidence  Against  Electrical  Stimulation  of  Postsynaptic  Po- 
tentials 

Mechanisms  of  Bioelectrogenesis 

Other  Consequences  of  Electrical  Inexcitability 

a)  Sustained  electrogenesis 

b)  Postsynaptic  potentials  during   hyperpolarization   and 
depolarization 

c)  Electrochemical  gradation  and  reversal  of  postsynaptic 
potentials 

d)  Latency  of  postsynaptic  potentials 

e)  Electrotonic  effects  of  presynaptic  impulse  upon  post- 
synaptic region 

f)  Chemical  sensitivity  of  synaptic  membrane 
Postsynaptic    Potentials    as   Nonpropagated     Standing'    Po- 
tentials 

Interaction  of  Graded  Responses 
Events  in  Synaptic  Transmission 

Functional  Interrelations  Within  Single  Cell 

Evolution  of  Electrogenic  Membrane 

Transmitter  Actions 

Genesis  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Gradation  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Mechanisms  of  Graded  Responsiveness 

Transfer  of  Activity  from   Postsynaptic   Potentials   to   Elec- 
trically Excitable  Membrane 

Synaptic  Delay 

Superposition  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials  and  Spikes 
General  and  Comparative  Physiology  of  Synapses 

Forms  and  Magnitudes  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 


'  The  researches  at  the  author's  laboratory  were  supported 
in  part  by  funds  from  the  following  sources :  Muscular  Dys- 
trophy Associations  of  America,  National  Institutes  of  Health 
(B-389  C),  National  Science  Foundation  and  United  Cerebral 
Palsy  Associations. 


Cells    with    Depolarizing    Postsynaptic    Po- 
Hyperpolarizing     Postsynaptic 


vith 


Postjunctional 
tentials 

Postjunctional 
Potentials 

Postjunctional   Cells   with   Both   Types   of  Postsynaptic   Po- 
tentials 

Fast  and  Slow  Responses  of  Invertebrate  Muscles 
Pharmacological  Properties  of  Synapses  and  their  Physiological 
Consequences 

Classification  of  Drug  Actions 

Identification  and  Characterization  of  Transmitter  Agents 

Modes  of  Action  of  Transmitter  Agents  and  Synaptic  Drugs 

Physiological  Implications 

a)  Topographic  distinctions 

b)  Synaptic  specificity  and  transmitters 

c)  Reciprocal  interactions  of  neural  pathways 

Role  of  Elementary  Synaptic  Properties  in  Integrative  Activity 
Spatial  Interrelations  of  Synaptic  and  Conductile  Membrane 
Physiological  Factors  Determining  Transmissional  Effective- 
ness 

a)  Synaptic  potency  and  drive 

b)  Excited  and  discharged  zones 

c)  Facilitation 

d)  Homosynaptic  facilitation 

e)  Heterosynaptic  facilitation 

f)  Spatial  summation  of  converging  pathways 
Integrative  Utility  of  Electrical  Inexcitability 
Synaptic  Determinants  of  Different  Types  of  Reflexes 
Role  of  Inhibition  in  Central  Nervous  System 
Physiological  Effects  of  Different  Porportions  of  Depolarizing 

and  Hyperpolarizing  Postsynaptic  Potentials 
Synaptic  Activity  and  Electrical  Concomitants 

a)  Interpretations  of  changes  in  amplitudes  of  postsynap- 
tic potentials 

b)  Interpretation  of  electrotonic  effects  of  standing  post- 
synaptic potentials 

c)  Synaptic  transducer  action  and  electrogenesis 
Ephaptic  Excitation 

Electrical  Modes  of  Transmission 
Role  of  Field  Currents  in  Central  Nervous  System 
Dorsal  Root  Reflex 

Ephaptic  Transmission   in   Annelid   and   Crustacean   Nerve 
Cords 
a)   Unpolarized  ephaptic  junctions 


■47 


148 


HANDBOC1K    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


b)  Polarized  ephaptic  transmission 
Evolutionary  Aspects  of  Ephaptic  Transmission 
Quasiartificial  Synapses 


CONTRACTION  OF  A  MUSCLE  when  an  apparently  un- 
reactive  nerve  is  stimulated,  the  problem  of  trans- 
mission in  its  most  obvious  form,  challenged  the  in- 
genuitv  of  early  physiologists.  Electricity  quickly 
became  a  relatively  familiar  force  after  the  invention 
of  the  Leyden  jar  and  was  inxoked  in  Galvani's 
theory  (84).  Electric  fluid  supplied  from  the  central 
nervous  system,  he  said,  charged  the  interior  of  a 
muscle  as  the  Leyden  jar  is  charged  by  an  electro- 
static machine.  Contraction  was  cau,sed  by  discharge 
of  this  electrical  fluid  when  the  mu.scle  and  its  nerve 
were  connected  by  a  metallic  arc.  The  'discharge 
hvpothesis'  formulated  by  Krause  and  Kiihne  in 
the  i86o's  encompassed  as  well  the  data  obtained 
in  the  two  decades  after  the  foundation  of  electro- 
physiology  by  du  Bois-Reymond  and  others.  "A 
nerve  onlv  throws  a  muscle  into  contraction  by 
means  of  its  currents  of  action,"  said  Kiihne  in  his 
Croonian  Lecture  of  1888  (133).  This  electric  theory 
of  transmis.sion  (fig.  i)  was  dominant  until  very  recent 
times  (98)  despite  the  questions  and  doubts  raised  by 
du  Bois-Reymond  himself  in  1874  (55),  and  by  Bern- 
stein in  1882  (20).  The  former  suggested  that  another 
inechanism,  secretion  by  the  nerve  of  some  chemical 
agent,  might  be  the  cause  of  neuromuscular  excita- 
tion. 

Transmission  in  the  central  nervous  system  hardly 
off"ered  a  problem  to  the  physiologists  of  the  nine- 
teenth century,  chiefly  for  one  reason.  Nerve  and 
muscle  are  distinctly  diff"erent  tissues  performing 
different  functions  and  obviously  joined  together  at  a 
specialized  region,  the  endplate.  Connections  between 
nerve  cells,  however,  were  thought  to  be  continuous, 
the  neurofibrils  of  one  penetrating  into  the  body  of 
another.  This  reticular  theory  of  Gerlach  was  chal- 
lenged only  at  the  end  of  the  nineteenth  century  when 
His,  Kolliker  and  pre-eminently  Ramon  y  Cajal 
proposed  the  neuron  theory  (169),  so  named  by  VVald- 
eyer.  Sherrington,  in  1897  (181),  applied  the  term 
synapse  to  the  region  of  contact  or  contiguity  at  which 
transmission  takes  place  from  the  presynaptic  nerve 
cell  to  another,  the  postsynaptic  cell.  The  present 
chapter  will  use  these  terms  in  their  general  context, 
including  in  this  sense  the  neuromuscular  and  neuro- 
glandular junctions. 

The  occurrence  of  demonstrai)le  barriers  at  the 
contacts  between  neurons,  different  staining  qualities 


B 


-_-:-  A£  .-,-._, 


■  ■  ^IV^'")  i^™^'^  il^^^i)  * ' 


FIG.  I.  Models  for  electrical  transmission.  .-1,  B.  du  Bois- 
Reymond's  'modified  discharge  hypothesis'  of  1874  for  the 
neuromuscular-  junction.  A:  The  current  loops  produced  at  a 
large  endplate  surface,  which  is  itself  not  part  of  the  muscle 
fiber,  he  thought  would  cause  both  anodal  and  cathodal  de- 
polarizations. The  current  fields,  indicated  by  the  arrows, 
would  thus  alternate  between  excitant  and  depressant  actions. 
B:  du  Bois-Reymond  suggested  that  a  geometrical  arrangement 
which  excited  the  muscle  at  a  point  contact  would  be  more 
effective.  [From  du  Bois-Reymond  (55).]  C.-  Eccles'  model  of 
1946  proposed  an  essentially  similar  arrangement.  Before  the 
impulse  of  the  presynaptic  fiber  had  arri\ed  at  the  synapse 
(left),  there  would  be  a  hyperpol  arizing  (inward)  current  flow 
in  the  synaptic  membrane.  When  the  impulse  reached  its 
terminus  (right)  it  would  cause  depolarization  and  excitation. 
[From  Eccles  (57).]  D:  Electrical  model  for  inhibitory  synaptic 
effects  showing  interaction  of  excitatory  (E)  and  inhibitory  (I) 
synapses.  The  latter  were  assumed  to  be  the  terminals  of  a  short 
axon,  Golgi  II  cell  which  developed  a  nonpropagating  spike  at 
its  soma.  The  anodal  focus  caused  by  the  I  knob  was  supposed 
to  depress  the  cathodal  excitatory  effects  of  the  E  knobs.  Cur- 
rent flows  are  simplified  in  the  diagram,  loops  which  are  sup- 
posed to  diminish  their  excitatory  effect  are  shown  only  at  the 
edges  of  each  E  knob.  [From  Brooks  et  at.  (28).] 


that  indicate  histochemical  differences  between  pre- 
and  postsynaptic  units  and  the  independent  existence 
of   the   latter   after   destruction    of   the   former   (i.e. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


■49 


absence  of  transneuronal  degeneration)  constituted 
the  evidence  brought  forward  by  Ramon  y  Cajal  and 
others  in  support  of  the  neuron  theory. 

When  the  neuron  theory  became  accepted,  the 
electrical  theory  of  transmission,  essentially  as  formu- 
lated by  Kiihne  for  the  neuromuscular  synapse,  was 
also  generally  adopted  (cf.  45,  57,  140).  Nevertheless, 
Sherrington's  life-long  study  of  the  central  nervous 
system  emphasized  that  the  physiological  actions  of 
the  latter  were  dominated  by  the  properties  of  syn- 
aptic transmission.  These,  he  thought  (183),  were  in 
many  respects  fundamentally  different  from  the 
properties  of  conductile  activity  of  nerve  or  muscle 
fibers,  in  which  all-or-none  impulses,  spikes,  are 
propagated  by  electrical  local  circuit  excitation 
within  the  confines  of  a  single  cell,  even  though  the 
latter  may  be  very  long  in  extent.  A  Russian  school  of 
physiology  headed  by  Ukhtom.sky  (cf  i  76)  ahso  main- 
tained that  central  nervous  phenomena  could  not  be 
explained  solely  in  terms  of  all-or-none  activity. 

The  neuron  theory  incorporates  and  gives  physio- 
logical meaning  to  the  doctrine  of  polarized  conduc- 
tion which  is  embodied  in  the  Bell-Magendie  Law. 
The  presynaptic  terminals  impinge  upon  the  synaptic, 
or  subsynaptic  (cf.  60)  membrane  of  the  postjunctional 
cell  with  various  types  of  contacts.  These  are  located 
chiefly,  but  not  exclusively,  at  the  dendrites  and  soma 
of  neurons,  and  Ramon  y  Cajal  distinguished  the 
different  sites  of  contact  as  axodendritic  and  axo- 
somatic  synapses  (cf.  1^9).  Contacts  between  the 
nerve  fibers  and  the  effector  cells,  muscle  or  gland, 
are  also  made  at  specialized  regions,  tho.se  of  mus- 
cle fibers  being  termed  endplates,  as  noted  above. 
Impulses  afferent  in  a  prefiber  evoke  activity  in  the 
postjunctional  cell.  If  the  cell  is  a  neuron,  its  junc- 
tional activity  may  result  in  a  spike  which  propa- 
gates along  the  latter's  axon.  At  the  terminals  of  this 
axon,  a  new  transfer  may  then  take  place  to  another 
neuron  or  to  an  effector  cell.  In  some  instances 
unidirectional  progression  is  apparently  invalidated, 
but  the  general  mechanism  of  these  cases  is  probably 
b\  ephaptic  transmission  (10).  This  appears  to  be 
fundamentally  different  from  synaptic  transmission 
and  will  be  discussed  in  the  last  section  of  this  chap- 
ter. One  recently  discovered  case  of  unidirectional 
conduction  (83)  produced  by  an  electrical  local  cir- 
cuit mechanism  will  also  be  discussed  at  that  time. 

The  concept  of  unidirectional  synaptic  transmission 
permitted  Ramon  y  Cajal  to  deduce  many  functional 
properties  of  the  central  nervous  system  from  anatom- 
ical data  (168).  Changes  that  occur  in  gross  and  fine 
structure,  in  histochemical  properties  and  in  physio- 


logical behavior  after  extirpation  or  damage  of 
specific  elements  also  give  clues  to  function.  The 
information  obtained  by  these  methods  relates  chiefly, 
however,  to  the  study  of  integrative  activity  which  is 
the  subject  of  later  chapters. 

While  it,  too,  bears  largely  on  integrative  functions, 
the  analysis  of  reflexes  as  exemplified  in  Sherrington's 
work  (cf  44,  182)  nevertheless  also  provides  data  on 
the  synaptic  processes  them.selves  and  discloses 
phenomena  such  as  cumulative,  long-lasting  excita- 
tory and  inhibitory  slates.  These  two  synaptic  prop- 
erties endow  the  central  nervous  system  with  its 
remarkable  flexibility  and  variety  of  responsiveness. 
Both  characteristics  may  also  be  present  in  simpler 
peripheral  synaptic  organizations  and  are  commonly 
found  in  the  peripheral  synaptic  structures  of  inverte- 
brates. Sherrington's  basic  method,  stimulation  of 
selected  pathways  and  study  of  their  effects  and  inter- 
actions, has  been  refined  by  application  of  modern 
electrophysiological  techniques.  The  combination 
has  given  information  on  the  effects  of  different  syn- 
aptic inflows,  their  relative  potencies,  the  temporal 
and  spatial  distribution  of  excitatory  and  inhibitory 
actions,  particularly  in  the  spinal  cord  (cf.  140;  and 
later  chapters  in  this  volume). 

The  electrophysiological  study  of  single  unit  path- 
ways such  as  nerve-muscle  or  neuron-neuron  provides 
still  more  detailed  and  intimate  information  on  synap- 
tic mechanisms  (cf.  62).  Microelectrode  recording, 
either  from  the  vicinity  of  single  cells  or  from  their 
interior,  is  a  recent  extension  of  the  technique  which 
can  provide  the  most  definitive  information  (52,  59, 
60,  95,  97).  In  all  cases,  transmissional  activity  is 
found  to  be  associated  with  a  special  type  of  electrical 
response,  the  postsynaptic  potential  or  p.s.p.  The 
transmissional  electrogenesis  at  the  endplates  of 
skeletal  muscle  fibers  is  known  as  the  endplate  po- 
tential (e.p.p.).  Basically,  however,  the  properties  of 
e.p.p.'s  arc  identical  with  those  of  p.s.p.'s.  A  presyn- 
aptic potential,  occurring  at  the  terminals  of  dorsal 
root  fibers,  has  also  been  described  ijut  from  indirect 
evidence  only  (140). 

Pharmacological  data  provide  much  of  the  oldest 
evidence  that  synaptic  transmis.sion  is  different  from 
the  conductile  process.  Claude  Bernard  (18)  found 
that  curare,  the  Indian  arrow  poison,  blocked  excita- 
tion of  a  muscle  by  its  nerve.  The  muscle  and  nerve 
individually  retain  their  conductile  properties,  and 
the  primary  effect  of  the  drug  is  on  the  transmission 
process.  Attempts  to  account  for  the  synaptic  blockade 
in  terms  of  electrical  transmission  were  not  successful 
(cf.  98).  A  host  of  other  chemicals  exert  actions  chiefly 


I50 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


wherever  synapses  occur.  These  junctional  regions 
also  appear  to  have  special  biochemical  requirements. 
Transmission,  for  example,  is  more  easily  disrupted 
by  anoxia  than  is  conduction.  Pharmacological  and 
biochemical  tools,  particularly  in  combination  with 
the  techniques  of  electrophysiology,  provide  additional 
data  on  the  processes  of  synaptic  transmission  (cf. 
96,  99-101,  161-166). 

A  challenge  to  the  electrical  theory  was  offered  by 
that  of  chemical  transmission  which  evolved  chiefly 
from  the  work  of  Dale,  Loewi,  Cannon  and  their 
associates  (cf.  150,  177).  According  to  this  view, 
activity  of  a  presynaptic  fiber  releases  at  its  synaptic 
terminals  a  chemical  transmitter  agent.  That  sub- 
stance excites  the  electrical  activity  of  the  postjunc- 
tional cell.  By  repetition  of  the  secretory  process  at 
the  terminals  of  the  latter,  a  new  action  is  started  in 
the  next  unit  of  a  transmissional  chain.  The  present 
chapter  adopts  this  view. 

The  conclusion  that  synaptic  transmission  obliga- 
torily involves  a  chemical  mediator  derives  from  a 
hypothesis  based  upon  a  recent  examination  of  data 
on  available  synaptic  systems  (97).  All  possess  a  com- 
mon constellation  of  properties  that  are  shown  in 
table  I  and  discussed  in  the  portion  of  this  chapter 
devoted  to  synaptic  electrogenesis.  The  entire  group 
of  these  distinguishing  characteristics  appears  to  be 
referable  to  a  single  fundamental  property  of  synaptic 
electrogenic  membrane,  namely  that  its  activity  is 
not  initiated  by  an  electrical  stimulus.  Thus,  there 
arises  a  profound  distinction  between  the  conductile 
activity  of  axons  or  muscle  fibers  and  the  transmis- 
sional activity  at  synapses.  The  former  is  electrically 
excitable  by  an  applied  stimulus  or  by  the  internally 
generated  local  circuit  of  activity.  The  latter  is  elec- 
trically ine.xcitable  and  must  be  evoked  by  a  specific 
stimulus  which  in  the  context  of  synaptic  structure 
must  be  a  chemical  excitant,  or  transmitter  agent, 
released  by  the  active  presynaptic  nerve  fibers. 

The  currently  used  definition  of  synapses  is  still 
essentially  as  it  developed  with  Sherrington  and 
Ramon  y  Cajal,  a  junction  in  contiguity  between 
anatomically  distinct  cells  across  which  activity  is 
nevertheless  transmitted,  but  only  in  one  direction, 
from  the  presynaptic  cell  to  the  postsynaptic.  Many 
other  specifications  are  now  available  to  distinguish 
transmissional  activity  from  conductile  or  ephaptic, 
and  these  appear  to  derive  from  the  one  feature,  that 
synaptic  activity  is  electrically  inexcitable. 

N.-VTURE  OF  PDSTSYN.\PTIC  PGTENTI.^LS 

The   earlier   studies    of   p.s.p.'s   were    made   with 


external  recordings  from  muscle  endplates  (59,  6q, 
63,  86),  sympathetic  ganglia  (56)  and  the  spinal  cord 
(57'  58)-  The  muscle  synapses  being  more  easily 
accessible,  it  was  most  intensively  studied  both 
electrophysiologically  and  pharmacologically  (cf. 
62).  More  recently,  this  and  many  other  varieties  of 
synapses  have  been  investigated  with  intracellular 
recording  (cf.  52,  59,  60,  68,  95,  97),  and  a  reasonably 
coherent  and  satisfactory  description  of  the  principles 
of  synaptic  electrogenesis  is  now  available. 

Generation  Sites  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

As  noted  above,  p.s.p.'s  are  associated  with  the 
occurrence  of  transmissional  activity  at  junctions 
between  a  pre-  and  a  postunit.  Only  in  a  few  systems 
(e.g.  neuromuscular  and  squid  giant  axon  synapses) 
is  the  junction  confined  to  a  clearly  delineated  area  of 
the  postunit.  In  these  cases  it  is  found  that  the  p.s.p. 
is  largest  within  the  region  of  the  junction  and  de- 
creases rapidly  as  the  distance  of  the  recording 
electrode  from  the  junction  increases  (fig.  2).  The 
form  of  the  potential  is  also  distorted  in  the  manner 
characteristic  of  electrotonic  spread  (114,  141),  both 
facts  indicating  that  the  site  at  which  electrogenesis 
occurs  is  confined  to  the  synaptic  region.  As  will  be 
described  below,  the  nonpropagating,  'standing' 
response  of  p.s.p.'s  is  a  consequence  of  electrical 
inexcitability. 

When  the  p.s.p.  is  recorded  with  a  microelectrode, 
at  first  externally  and  then  internally,  the  sign  of  the 
p.s.p.  reverses  when  the  electrode  penetrates  the  cell. 
Like  the  spike,  which  also  undergoes  reversal  of  sign 
under  the  same  conditions,  the  neurally  evoked  po- 
tential is  produced  at  the  excitable  electrogenic 
membrane  of  the  postjunctional  cell,  hence  the  term 
p.s.p. 

Molecular  Structures  of  Differently  Excitable  Membranes 

The  structures  of  the  membranes  that  are  involved 
in  ssnaptic  activity  are  not  as  yet  known.  The  pre- 
synaptic terminals  occur  in  an  immense  variety  of 
shapes  and  sizes.  In  some  of  these  electron  microscopy 
has  indicated  the  presence  of  vesicles  (54,  174).  The 
latter  have  been  interpreted  (cf.  52)  as  sites  of  concen- 
tration of  chemical  mediators  which  presumably  are 
formed  in  the  nerve  fibers  and  ejected  during  activity 
into  an  extracellular  synaptic  space  of  about  100  A. 
The  postsynaptic  sites  which  respond  specifically  to 
the  chemical  transmitter  agents  cannot,  at  present,  be 
differentiated  structurally  from  those  of  electrically 
excitable  membranes.  This  is  perhaps  l^est  exemplified 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


FIG.  2.  Some  properties  of  depolarizing  postsynaptic  poten- 
tials. .-1,  B:  The  intracellularly  recorded  e.p.p.  of  a  mammalian 
muscle  fiber  is  evoked  by  neural  stimuli  during  hyperpolariza- 
tion  of  the  muscle  fiber  membrane  through  another  intracellu- 
lar electrode.  The  impaled  hyperpolarized  fiber  did  not  re- 
spond with  a  spike  or  contraction,  but  others  unaffected  by  the 
polarizing  current  and  excited  by  the  neural  \olley  contracted. 
The  resulting  movement  pulled  the  microelectrode  out  of  the 
tested  muscle  fiber  producing  the  artifact  seen  at  the  end  of 
each  record.  The  response  in  B  is  smaller  than  that  in  A, 
partly  because  it  is  generated  at  a  less  hyperpolaiized  membrane 
as  is  described  in  the  text.  However,  it  is  also  broader  than  the 
response  in  A,  indicating  that  the  recording  microelectrode  was 
probably  some  distance  from  the  focus  of  the  e.p.p.  The  effects 
of  recording  at  various  distances  from  this  focus  are  shown  in 
C,  D  and  E.  The  amplitude  of  the  e.p.p.  falls  sharply  (C);  the 
rising  phase  is  prolonged  somewhat  (i))  and  the  falling  phase 
even  more  (£)  as  the  electrode  is  moved  farther  from  the  focus. 
[From  Boyd  &  Martin  (23).] 


by  electron  microscopic  studies  of  eel  electroplaques 

(95). 

These  cells  possess  three  functionally  distinct  types 
of  membrane.  One  major  surface  is  composed  of 
membrane  that  does  not  respond  electrogenically  to 
any  type  of  stimulation  and  has  a  very  low  electrical 
resistance.  The  other  major  surface  of  each  cell  is 
diffusely  innervated  and,  presumably  only  under  the 
presynaptic  terminals,  there  is  excitable  membrane 
of  the  synaptic  type  which  responds  only  to  neural  or 
to  chemical  stimuli.   Intermingled  with  this  electri- 


cally inexcitable  membrane  component  is  one  that  is 
electrically  excitable  and  produces  a  spike.  Electron 
microscopy  has  as  yet  not  been  able  to  discern  differ- 
ences between  the  two  different  components  of  the 
excitable  membrane,  nor  between  their  structures  and 
those  of  the  nonresponsive  membrane  (95,  143). 

Two  functionally  quite  different  junctions,  in  squid 
and  crayfish  respectively,  appear  to  be  similar  when 
observed  by  electron  microscopy  C'75)-  However, 
that  activating  the  giant  axon  of  squid  is  electrically 
ine.xcitable  and  thus  conforms  to  the  extended  defini- 
tion of  synapses  given  above.  On  the  other  hand,  the 
junction  between  a  medial  giant  fiber  and  the  motor 
giant  axon  of  the  crayfish  (83),  as  will  be  discussed 
below,  appears  to  resemble  the  ephaptic  junctions  of 
septate  giant  axons  (125). 

The  inai)ility  of  present  day  microscopic  techniques 
to  differentiate  the  structures  of  membranes  which 
differ  profoundly  in  their  functional  properties  indi- 
cates that  the  differences  which  determine  these 
properties  must  be  at  the  molecular  level.  Probably, 
as  microscopic  methods  develop,  the  difficulty  of 
visualizing  molecular  differences  will  be  overcome. 
At  present,  however,  the  chief  tools  available  for 
analyzing  these  structures  are  electrophysiological 
obser\ations  of  function  and  of  the  disturbance  in 
function  produced  by  various  experimental  means, 
including  the  use  of  chemical  agents  (cf.  gg-ioi;  163). 

Types  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

•Synaptic  electrogenesis  differs  from  that  of  the 
spike  by  being  relatively  small  and,  when  more  than 
one  nerve  fiber  is  available  to  excite  it,  is  graded  in 
amplitude  depending  on  the  strength  of  the  stimulus 
to  the  nerve.  Furthermore,  two  varieties  of  p.s.p.'s  can 
occur.  One,  like  the  spike,  tends  to  decrease  the  resting 
potential,  hence  is  a  depolarizing  p.s.p.  The  other 
tends  to  increase  the  resting  potential  and  is  therefore 
a  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  The  two  varieties  of  p.s.p.'s 
are  present  in  different  proportions  in  different  cells. 
Some  cells  generate  only  depolarizing,  others  only 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s,  while  in  a  third  group  both 
types  of  responses  are  produced  usually,  and  perhaps 
always,  by  stimulation  of  different  neural  inflows.  All 
vertebrate  muscle  fibers  thus  far  known,  their  em- 
bryological  relatives  the  electroplaques  of  most  elec- 
tric organs  and  some  neurons  develop  only  a  depolar- 
izing p.s.p.  Certain  gland  cells  are  at  present  known 
in  which  a  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  is  the  sole  electro- 
genesis  (144,  146).  The  crayfish  stretch  receptor, 
likewise,  produces  a  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  C'So),  but 


152 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


depolarizing  electrogenesis  is  also  evoked,  although  in 
this  case  by  stretch  of  the  mechanosensory  receptor 
membrane  (66,  67,  94).  In  other  cells,  notably  neurons 
of  the  vertebrate  central  nervous  system  (59,  60,  158, 
159,  1 61-16  7)  and  some  invertebrate  muscle  fibers 
(73.  13O  and  neurons  (33,  186),  l:)oth  types  of  p.s.p.'s 
are  found. 

The  depolarizing  p.s.p.,  being  of  the  same  sign  as 
the  effective  stimulus  for  electrical  or  local  circuit 
production  of  a  spike,  can  also  evoke  the  latter  and  is 
therefore  termed  an  " excitatory'  p.s.p.  (59,  60).  The 
spike  arises  when  the  p.s.p.  is  sufficient  to  depolarize 
the  adjacent  electrically  excitable,  spike-generating 
membrane  to  a  critical  firing  level  (fig.  3).  The  latter 
varies  among  different  cells  and  is  of  the  order  of  10 
to  40  mv  change  from  the  resting  level.  The  hyper- 
polarizing  p.s.p.,  by  the  same  criterion,  is  an  '  in- 
hibitorv'  p.s.p.  However,  these  names  are  not  always 
appropriate.  There  are  cells,  like  some  electroplaques 
or  muscle  fibers  (cf.  95,  97),  that  generate  depolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  but  no  spikes.  The  depolarizing  p.s.p.  there- 


fore may  have  nothing  electrogenic  to  excite.  Like- 
wise, those  gland  cells  which  generate  only  hyper- 
polarizing  p.s.p.'s  al.so  have  no  spike  to  inhibit  (cf. 
fig.  20).  On  the  contrary,  the  hyperpolarizing  electro- 
genesis  of  the  gland  cells  is  associated  with  actixity  in 
the  form  of  secretion  (146). 

When  the  two  varieties  of  p.s.p.'s  occur  in  a  cell 
\shich  also  generates  spikes,  they  interplay  with 
excitatory  and  inhiljitory  influences  upon  the  elec- 
trically excitable  membrane.  The  inhibitory  synaptic 
action  may  occur  independently  of  the  magnitude 
and  even  tiie  sign  of  the  inhibitory  p.s.p.  As  will  be 
descriljed  below  (p.  160)  this  p.s.p.  may  be  de- 
polarizing under  certain  electrochemical  conditions, 
or  the  acti\'ity  of  the  synaptic  membrane  may  not 
manifest  itself  as  a  potential.  Nevertheless,  when  this 
synaptic  activity  is  pitted  against  a  depolarizing 
p.s.p.  it  always  tends  to  decrease  the  magnitude  of 
the  latter  and  thereby  to  diminish  or  block  its  ex- 
citatory effect  on  the  electrically  excitaljle  membrane. 
In  some  cases,  therefore,  the  term  "inhibitory"  p.s.p 


msec 


msec 


FIG.  3.  Synaptic  transfer  from  the  p.s.p.  to  the  spike.  Intracellulai"  recording,  eel  electroplaque. 
Above:  Increasing  stimuU  to  a  nerve  produced  a  stepwise  increase  of  the  p.s.p.  (.4  to  C).  A  still  larger 
stimulus  evoked  a  spike  (Z)  and  £).  Below:  The  p.s.p.  first  generates  a  local,  graded  response  of  the 
electrically  excitable  spike-generating  membrane.  When  the  neural  stimulus  evokes  a  p.s.p.  during 
the  absolute  refractory  period  {A',  B'),  the  response  lacks  this  component  of  giaded  activity  of  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane.  Later  (C  to  G")  the  local  response  develops,  grows,  arises  earlier 
and  fuses  with  the  p.s.p.  The  combined  response  is  seen  in  isolation  in  H'.  This  series  of  records  was 
taken  at  approximately  '  1 0  the  amplification  of  the  upper  set.  Baseline  denotes  the  zero  for  the 
resting  potential  and  for  the  overshoot  of  the  spikes.  [From  Altamirano  el  al.  (4).] 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


153 


is  more  apt  than  'hyperpolarizing'  p.s.p.,  btit  the 
lattei  term  may  be  extended  to  denote  a  tendency 
to  maintain  as  well  as  to  increase  the  resting  potential. 

Interrelations  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials  and  Spikes 

It  has  been  noted  above  that  the  p.s.p.  is  not  ac- 
tively propagated  as  is  the  spike.  Thus,  the  transmis- 
sional  electrogenesis  of  a  p.s.p.  is  confined  to  the  syn- 
aptic site.  While  their  local  electrical  activity  can  be 
recorded  in  or  about  the  cells  that  produce  it  (cf. 
51,  70),  p.s.p.'s  do  not,  in  general,  evoke  activit\'  in 
other  cells,  their  effects  being  confined  to  the  cell  in 
which  they  originate. 

To  elicit 'distant'  actions  in  the  next  postjtinctional 
cell,  the  prejunctional  cell  must  generate  a  spike. 
Thus,  transmissional  activity  in  a  synaptically  linked 
chain  of  units  is  consummated  only  if  the  p.s.p.  of  each 
unit  evokes  a  spike.  When  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.  in 
one  of  the  linked  elements  is  insufficient  to  elicit  a 
spike,  the  transmissional  chain  is  broken.  Likewise,  if 
at  one  synaptic  site,  inhibitory  p.s.p.  is  sufficiently 
large  to  block  the  spike  of  that  cell,  the  chain  is  also 
broken. 

Thus,  spikes  and  p.s.p.'s  are  functionally  interre- 
lated. The  former  command  the  secretory  activity  at 
presynaptic  terminals  of  their  cell,  and  the  released 
transmitter  agent  then  evokes  the  p.s.p.  of  the  next 
cell,  which  may  or  may  not  itself  elicit  a  new  spike,  to 
repeat  the  process.  It  should  be  noted  that  while 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  can  inhibit  spike  production, 
they  are  themselves  evoked  by  an  excitatory  activity 
in  the  presynaptic  cell  that  propagates  within  the 
latter  and  effects  the  hyperpolarization  of  the  post- 
junctional synaptic  membrane  through  the  secretory 
activity  that  it  calls  forth  in  the  presynaptic  terminals. 
In  other  words,  a  p.s.p.,  whether  excitatory  or 
inhibitory,  always  represents  an  active  process,  a 
response  of  subsynaptic  membrane  to  an  appropriate 
excitant. 

As  was  noted  abo\e,  and  will  be  descriiied  in  more 
detail  below,  the  electrically  inexcitable  synaptic 
electrogenic  membrane  has  different  properties  from 
those  which  generate  the  spike.  The  properties  even  of 
simple  synaptic  s\stems  are  therefore  compounded 
from  and  subject  to  the  various  properties  of  the  differ- 
ent electrogenic  components.  The  multiplicity  of  syn- 
aptic transfers  in  the  central  nervous  system  makes  the 
synaptic  properties  a  dominant  factor,  although  those 
of  conductile  electrogenesis  are  also  important.  Since 
the  amount  and  type  of  synaptic  electrogenesis  deter- 


mines the  occurrence  or  absence  of  spikes,  factors 
which  modify  p.s.p.'s  are  therefore  of  sjreat  significance 
in  the  central  nervous  system.  Among  these  are  the 
effects  of  pharmacological  agents  or  synaptic  drugs, 
and  their  use  as  experimental  tools  has  already  been 
mentioned.  However,  other  agents  and  physiological 
conditions  may  affect  production  of  p.s.p.'s.  For  ex- 
ample, the  synaptic  membrane  may  be  altered  in  its 
properties  by  previous  activity  (cf  95,  97;  and  below) 
and  this  could  affect  synaptic  electrogenesis.  The 
physiological  properties  of  the  presynaptic  terminals 
may  also  be  changed  by  various  conditions,  including 
previous  activity.  This  change  might  affect  the  amount 
or  nature  of  the  transmitter  agent  released  under  the 
new  circumstances  and  thereby  aflfect  transmission. 
Thus,  the  magnesium  ion  interferes  with  release  of 
transmitter  agents  from  the  presynaptic  terminals 
(cf.  52).  Neuromuscular  transmission  is  then  depressed 
or  blocked.  The  calcium  ion  acts  reciprocally  and  in 
excess  antagonizes  the  effects  of  excess  magnesium 
ion. 


SPECIFIC  PROPERTIES  OF  SYN.APTIC  ELECTROGENESIS 

Evidence    Against    Electrical    Stimulation    of 
Postsynaptic   Potentials 

The  existence  of  varieties  of  postjunctional  cells  in 
which  p.s.p.'s  are  generated  without  spikes,  e.g.  in 
Torpedo  and  Raia  electroplaques,  invertebrate  and 
vertebrate  muscle  fibers  and  gland  cells  (cf.  95,  97), 
provides  one  kind  of  direct  evidence  for  electrical 
inexcitability  of  synaptic  membrane  (figs.  4/I,  5;  cf. 
fig.  20).  An  electrical  stimulus  which  does  not  fire  the 
presynaptic  nerve  fibers  evokes  no  electrical  activity 
in  these  cells.  Responses  are  only  produced  by  afferent 
neural  activity  or  by  chemicals  which  thus  mimic  the 
action  of  the  transmitter  agent  (fig.  5). 

Even  in  those  cells  which  also  generate  spikes,  the 
p.s.p.  is  produced  only  by  neural  or  chemical  stimuli. 
Direct  electrical  stimuli  applied  to  the  cell,  or  its  local 
circuit  excitation  by  antidromic  invasion,  evoke  only 
spikes  without  p.s.p.'s  (fig.  6).  Finally,  the  occurrence 
of  spikes  and  of  absolute  refractoriness  which  is  their 
concomitant  does  not  preclude  the  independent 
development  of  p.s.p.'s.  The  electrogenesis  of  the 
latter  then  can  be  superimposed  upon  that  of  the 
spike,  i.e.  it  can  be  evoked  during  the  absolute  refrac- 
tory period  (figs.  6,  7).  Together  therefore,  these  three 
types  of  data  provide  direct  evidence  that  the  p.s.p.'s 
are  generated  by  membrane  that  is  not  itself  electri- 


'54 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


^ 


TABLE  I.  Characteristics  and  Properties  of  Differently 
Excitable  Electrogenic  Membrane 


FIG.  4.  Differences  between  electrically  inexcitable  and  ex- 
citable membrane.  A :  The  slow  muscle  fiber  of  the  frog  is  not 
electrically  excitable  and  produces  no  spikes,  even  when  the 
membrane  is  strongly  depolarized  at  beginning  of  (a).  It 
develops  p.s.p.  on  stimulation  of  the  nerve  during  the  applica- 
tion of  the  electrical  pulse.  The  response  at  the  resting  potential 
(«),  a  depolarizing  p.s.p.,  is  increased  when  the  membrane  is 
hyperpolarized  by  the  applied  pulse  (/,  g).  The  p.s.p.  is  de- 
creased by  depolarizing  the  membrane  (</)  and  is  reversed  in 
direction  by  strongly  depolarizing  the  membrane  (a  to  f)-  The 
magnitude  of  the  reversed  depolarizing  p.s.p.  increases  as  the 
interior  of  the  membrane  is  driven  beyond  an  equilibrium  po- 
tential given  approximately  by  the  pulse  in  c.  [From  Burke  & 
Ginsborg  (35).]  B:  Responses  of  a  cat  motoneuron  to  ortho- 
dromic stimuli  show  essentially  the  same  behavior  of  the  p.s.p.'s, 
but  are  complicated  by  the  appearance  of  a  spike  and  the  inac- 
tivation  of  electrically  excitable  membrane.  The  response  at  the 
resting  potential  (  —  66  mv)  is  a  depolarizing  p.s.p.  which  does 
not  elicit  a  spike.  Hyperpolarization  of  the  membrane  caused 
little  change  in  the  p.s.p.  Depolarizations  to  —60  mv  and  —42 
mv  summed  with  the  excitatory  effect  of  the  p.s.p.,  evoking 
spikes.  These  are  no  longer  produced  by  the  p.s.p.'s  at  the  rest- 
ing potential  —  32  mv,  etc.  These  depolarizations,  after  evoking 
spikes  by  the  electrical  stimuli,  then  inactivated  the  spike- 
generating  membrane.  The  p.s.p.'s  decreased  and  at  a  mem- 
brane potential  of  +3  mv  disappeared  but  reappeared  in 
reversed  sign  as  the  internal  face  of  the  membrane  was  made 
more  positive.  [From  Eccles  (60).] 

cally  excitable.  Other  properties  of  p.s.p  's  that  dis- 
tinguish them  from  spikes  are  also  referable  to  this 


Spike 
(Electrical^  Excitable) 


P.s.p.'s 

(Electrically  Inexcitable) 


A.  Characterisii 
Transducer  action; 

(i)  Sequential  increase  of 
Na^  and  K+  conduct- 
ances and  Na*  inactiva- 
tion 


(ii)  Rates     determined     by 
membrane  potential 
Electrical  response: 
(i)  Begins  with  graded  de- 
polarization,     develops 
overshoot 
(ii)  All-or-none  response 


Two  types: 

a)  increased  conductances 

for  all  ions 
A)  specific  increase  in  K+ 
and/or     Cl~     conduct- 
ances 
Rates     not     determined     by 
membrane  potential 

Two  types: 

a)  depolarizing 

6)  hyperpolarizing 
Graded  response 


B.  Direct  Evidence  Jor  Characteristic  Differences 

Developed  only  by  neural  or 


(i)  Spike  absent  | 
(ii)  Spike  presentj 


chemical  stimuli 


C.  Consequences  of  Characteristic  Differences 


(i)  Always   in   depolarizing 
direction 

(ii)  Hindered  or  blocked  by 
hyperpolarization 

(iii)  Excited,     then     blocked 
by  depolarization 

(iv)  Pulsatile,  relatively 

fixed      duration      inde- 
pendent of  stimulus 
(v)  "Vanishingly     brief     la- 
tency 

(vi)  Relatively        inert        to 
chemicals 


(vii)  Decrementless  propaga- 
tion 


Of  either  sign,  electrochemi- 

cally  reversible 
Electrochemical  gradation 

Electrochemical  gradation 

May  be  prolonged,  sustained 
while  stimulus  lasts 

Appreciable,  irreducible  la- 
tency 

Sensitive    in    two    ways:    re- 
sponse may  be 
a)  evoked  by  synapse  acti- 
vators; 
i)  depressed     or     blocked 
by  inactivators 

Nonpropagated,      'standing' 
potential 


single,  fundamental  difference  in  their  modes  of 
excitation.  These  correlations  are  summarized  in  table 
I,  and  form  the  content  of  this  section  (cf.  also  97). 


Mechanisms  of  Bioelectrogenesis 

The  means  by  which  a  cell  can  generate  electrical 
activity  are  restricted  in  variety  by  the  nature  of  the 
physiological  and  electrochemical  systems  of  living 
tissues  (91,  112,  113).  Conductile  and  transmissional 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION  1 55 

A    -  r\       B 


FIG.  5.  Electrogenic  action  of  acetylcholine  on  the  elec- 
trically inexcitable  membrane  of  Torpedo  elcctroplaques.  Intra- 
arterial injections  of  10  /ng  (/),  5  /ig  (//)  and  2.5  /ig  (/!')  in  the 
presence  of  physostigmine  produced  electrical  activity,  the 
larger  amounts  evoking  the  larger  responses.  The  neurally 
evoked  discharge  of  Torpedo  organ  lasts  only  a  few  msec.  (cf. 
95).  The  long  duration  of  the  response  produced  by  injections 
of  acetylcholine  presumably  is  due  to  sustained  depolarization 
of  the  electrically  inexcitable  elcctroplaques  by  an  excess  of  the 
administered  transmitter  agent.  ///  indicates  a  control  in  which 
only  perfusion  fluid  was  injected.  The  elcctroplaques  were 
probably  depolarized  in  the  'resting'  state,  and  the  'hyper- 
polarization'  seen  in  this  record  may  have  been  caused  by 
temporary  dilution  of  the  depolarizing  e.vcitant.  Calibrations: 
0.5  mv,  and  seconds.  [From  Feldberg  &  Fessard  (74).] 


excitable  membranes  utilize  the  electrical  polarization 
or  resting  potential  of  the  cell.  This  appears  as  a 
potential  difference  across  the  cell  membrane  with  its 
interior  negative  relative  to  the  exterior.  At  rest,  the 
membrane  has  a  rather  high  resistance,  indicating 
that  it  presents  a  considerable  barrier  to  the  penetra- 
tion of  ions.  The  physiological  electrogcnic  response 
of  the  membrane  to  an  appropriate  stimulus,  its 
transducer  action  (94),  is  the  temporary  alteration  of 
its  permittivity  to  ions.  The  electrical  change  is  its 
consequence,  derived  from  the  prior,  metabolically 
energized  unequal  distribution  of  ions  and  the  resting 
potential. 

Whereas  the  spike  is  generated  by  temporally 
sequential  processes  comprising  first  enhanced  sodium 
conductance,  then  enhanced  potassium  conductance 
and  sodium  inactivation(ii3),^  the  transducer  actions 
of  svnaptic  membrane  involve  different  ionic  events. 

-  Recent  data  on  eel  elcctroplaques  (3)  indicate  that  a  process 
of  potassium  inactivation  may  be  involved  in  spike  production 
(95).  The  participation  of  other,  potential-insensitive  processes 
is  discussed  below  in  connection  with  graded  responses  of 
electrically  excitable  membrane. 


FIG.  6.  Some  differences  between  electrically  and  neurally 
excitable  responses.  A,  B:  Weak  and  strong  depolarizing  elec- 
trical stimuli  to  the  eel  electroplaque  excited  the  cell  directly, 
the  latter  with  almost  no  latency.  C,  D:  The  stimuli  were  ap- 
plied in  the  reverse  direction.  These  are  ineffective  for  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane  but  stimulate  the  cell  indirectly 
by  way  of  the  nerve  terminals  supplying  the  synaptic  mem- 
brane. The  weak  indirect  stimulus  evoked  only  a  p.s.p.  after  a 
latency  of  almost  2  msec.  (C).  The  very  strong  stimulus  (i)) 
shortened  the  latency  to  about  1.7  msec,  and  the  larger  p.s.p. 
evoked  a  spike  with  very  brief  delay.  No  p.s.p.'s  were  produced 
by  the  direct  stimuli.  However,  the  strong  direct  stimulus  (B) 
also  excited  the  nerve  fibers  which  csokcd  a  p.s.p.  that  occurred 
with  the  same  latency  as  in  C  and  D  but  appearing  this  time  on 
the  falling  phase  of  the  directly  elicited  spike.  The  p.s.p.  there- 
fore occurred  while  the  electrically  excitable  membrane  was 
absolutely  refractory.  [From  Altamirano  et  al.  (4).] 


Depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  are  caused  by  a  general  increa.se 
of  permittivity  to  all  ions  (71 ;  cf.  52,  60)  which  tends 
to  abolish  the  resting  potential.  Electrogenesis  of 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  probably  involves  increased 
permittivity  for  K+  and  Cl^  (60,  61;  Grundfest 
el  al.,  in  preparation).  Each  ion  species  then  moves  in 
the  direction  of  its  electrochemical  gradient,  K+ 
outward  and  Cl^  inward.  Loss  of  positive  charges  and 
gain  of  negative  thus  account  for  the  increased 
internal  negativity. 

The  immediate  consequences  of  electrical  inexcita- 
bility  of  synaptic  transducer  actions  are  made  appar- 
ent by  the  diagram  of  figure  8.  Depolarization  is  the 
stimulus  that  initiates  transducer  action  of  an  elec- 
trically excitable  membrane.  The  entry  of  Na""", 
forced  inward  because  of  the  high  concentration  of 
this  ion  in  the  external  medium,  causes  further  de- 
polarization. This  electrogenic  response  to  the  trans- 


156  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7.  Absence  of  refractoriness  in  postsyn- 
aptic responses  in  the  giant  neuron  of  Aplysia. 
A :  A  single  shock  to  the  presynaptic  nerve  first 
evokes  a  long-lasting  p.s.p.  out  of  which  rises 
the  spike  of  the  giant  neuron.  B:  A  second  stim- 
ulus, exciting  the  cell  during  its  refractory  pe- 
riod, adds  a  potential  (solid  line  beginning  at 
arrow)  to  the  initial  response  (broken  line). 
The  difference  (dotted  line,  below)  is  due  to 
the  second  p.s.p.  C:  The  second  stimulus  was 
delivered  somewhat  later.  The  added  potential 
also  shows  a  local  response  (prep.")  which  was 
initiated  by  the  p.s.p.  in  the  electrically  ex- 
citable membrane  during  the  relatively  refrac- 
tory period.  D:  At  a  longer  interval,  a  second 
stimulus  evokes  the  full  response  as  in  A.  [Froin 
Arvanitaki  &  Chalazonites  (11).] 


B 


/>■»/» 


looms 


ducer  action  of  the  membrane  can  then  act  as  a  further 
stimulus  to  the  latter.  The  positive  feedback  of  the 
effect  leads  to  a  regenerative  sequence  and  to  the 
explosive,  all-or-none  spike.  Since  the  transducer 
actions  of  electrically  inexcitable  membrane  are  not 
affected  by  the  electrogenesis  of  the  p.s.p.'s,  feedback 
either  positive  (in  the  case  of  the  depolarizing),  or 
negative  (for  the  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s)  is  lacking. 
Because  of  the  absence  of  electrical  feedback  p.s.p.'s 
of  either  sign  are  thus  produced  that  are  graded  in 
proportion  to  the  availability  of  the  specific  excitants 
of  the  respective  transducer  actions. 

Ot/wr  Consequences  of  Electrical  Incxcitability 

a)  susT.oiiNED  ELECTROGENESIS.  Thc  transducer  actions 
of  the  spike  generator  are  a  sequence  of  potential- 
determined  events,  the  first  (.sodium  conductance) 
tending  to  cause  the  depolarizing  electrogenesis, 
others  (potassium  conductance,  sodium  inactivation) 
tending  to  terminate  it  and  to  restore  the  resting 
potential.  The  sensitivity  of  these  processes  to  the 
changes  in  membrane  potential  produced  by  the 
electrogenesis  itself  thus  leads  to  a  self-limiting  event, 
the  spike,  of  rather  constant  duration  with  which  is 
also  associated  refractoriness  (113).  Not  being  elec- 
tricallv  excitable,  the  transducer  actions  of  the    syn- 


aptic membrane  are  relatively  in,sensitive  to  the 
changes  of  membrane  potential.  Hence,  p.s.p.'s 
may  be  sustained  as  long  as  the  excitant  of  the  trans- 
ducer action  is  available  (fig.  g)  since  they  are  not 
subject  to  refractoriness  (figs.  6,  7)  nor  inactivation. 
The  transducers  of  most  types  of  sensory  membrane 
are  probably  also  electrically  inexcitable  (94,  95,  97). 
The  sustained  graded  electrogenesis  which  can  de- 
velop to  a  sustained  stimulus  is  the  means  for  trans- 
mitting information  by  a  train  of  pulsatile  spikes, 
coded  as  to  frequency  and  number  in  some  relation 
to  the  intensity  and  duration  of  the  stimulus  (97,  103; 
fig.  10;  cf.  fig.  13).  The  transducers  of  some  mechano- 
sensitive  organs,  at  least,  also  have  chemical  sensitivity 
(94,  96,  97),  indicating  further  their  relations  with 
chemically  sensitive  synaptic  membrane. 

Although  the  postsynaptic  membrane,  in  contrast 
to  the  electrically  excitable,  is  capable  of  sustained 
electrogenesis,  its  responsiveness  to  a  steady  stimulus 
mav  be  affected  in  various  ways.  These  reflect  the 
labilitN  of  thc  membrane  in  the  face  of  the  very  chemi- 
cal agents  by  which  it  is  excited  (95,  96).  An  example 
is  the  gradual  diminution  or  even  disappearance  of 
synaptic  electrogenesis  when  a  muscle  or  autonomic 
ganglion  is  continuously  acted  upon  h\  acet\  Icholine 
or  other  agents  (123,  127,  129,  187). 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


157 


Stimulus 

Transducer  Action 

(Increosed  Memtifor^e 
Conduclonce) 

Electrogenesis        Response 

Electrical 

(Depolarizing) 

—          No*               -^ 

Depolarizotion  — •  Spike  ond 

decrementless 

Chemicol 

►     General           — ► 

Depolonzotion  — •  Graded  excitotory 
posl-synoptic 
potential 

Chemical 

►  K+and/orCr  — ► 

Hyper-              — ►  Groded  inhibitory 
polorizotion              post -synoptic 
potential 

FIG.  8.  The  different  ionic  mechanisms  evoked  by  transducer 
actions  in  electrically  excitable  and  synaptic  membranes,  and 
some  consequences  of  the  different  excitabilities.  The  depolari- 
zation caused  by  an  electrical  stimulus  is  regenerative  in  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane  and  produces  the  all-or-none 
spike.  The  electrically  inexcitable  synaptic  membrane  can 
produce  either  depolarizing  or  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  which 
do  not  react  back  on  the  transducer  actions.  This  insensitivity 
to  electrical  effects  results  in  responses  graded  in  proportion  to 
the  available  chemical  stimulus.  The  depolarizing  p.s.p.  can 
act  as  a  stimulus  for  the  electrically  excitable  membrane,  while 
the  hyperpolarizing  is  inhibitory  to  the  latter.  [From  Grund- 
fest  (96).] 


The  kinetics  of  this  reversible  desensitization  have 
been  studied  thus  far  only  in  frog  muscle  endplates 
(fig.  11).  The  nature  of  the  processes  involved  C127) 
is  not  yet  clear;  but  neither  the  loss  of  responsivene.ss 
nor  its  recovery  are  controlled  by  the  membrane  po- 
tential. 

Desensitization  may  be  slow  and  unimportant 
relative  to  the  excitatory  events  that  occur  at  synapses 
in  response  to  their  normal  neural  activation.  How 
ever,  it  might  become  a  disturbing  factor  if  trans- 
mitters are  continuously  released  locally  or  svstem- 
ically.  This  situation  could  result  from  the  action  of 
drugs  or  might  arise  from  a  pathological  state.  Rapidly 
developing  desensitization  has  not  yet  been  described, 
but  it  might  account  for  the  successively  decreased 
p.s.p.'s  sometimes  produced  by  a  train  of  stimuli. 
This  process  has  been  termed  'defacilitation'  (33, 
186).  Decrease  in  the  generator  potential  of  sense 
organs  acted  upon  by  a  constant  stimulus,  such  as  is 
seen  in  the  rapidly  adapting  stretch  receptors  of  cray- 
fish (66),  might  be  accounted  for  by  a  desensitization 
phenomenon. 


100  msec 


i—r 


15  msec 
T  M  M 


FIG.  g.  Soine  consequences  of  the  differently  excitable  electrogenic  mechanisms  in  neurons,  a.' 
The  cat  motoneuron  excited  antidromically  at  high  frequencies  (140,  205,  280  and  630  per  sec.) 
produces  pulsatile  spikes,  only  their  after -potentials  fusing.  [From  Brock  et  al.  (25).]  b:  The  p.s.p.'s 
produced  by  orthodromic  stimuli  (205  and  280  per  sec.)  summate,  a  higher  average  level  of  the  de- 
polarization being  produced  by  the  higher  frequency  of  stimulation.  The  summated  response  is 
maintained  as  long  as  the  afferent  stimuli  are  delivered  (lower  record  of  each  set).  The  amplitude 
calibration  applies  to  the  p.s.p.'s  of  this  .set  which  were  taken  at  about  lox  the  amplification  of  a. 
[From  Brock  et  al.  (24).]  c:  Repetitive  activity  evoked  in  the  rabbit  cervical  syinpathetic  neuron  by 
stimulating  the  preganglionic  supply  at  approximately  80,  100,  120  and  150  per  sec.  At  the  time 
scale  of  the  records  the  first  p.s.p.  is  not  shown  (cf.  fig.  17C).  The  p.s.p.  evokes  a  large  spike;  but 
even  at  the  lowest  frequency,  the  spikes  caused  by  the  subsequent  p.s.p.'s  are  small,  while  the  p.s.p.'s 
themselves  are  summed  and  sustained.  This  synaptic  depolarization,  increasing  at  higher  frequencies 
of  afferent  drive,  inactivates  the  spike-generating  membrane.  \i\.er  the  second  depressed  spike  the 
responses  progressively  decrease,  and  at  the  highest  frequency  disappear.  The  p.s.p.'s  are  generated 
as  long  as  there  is  an  influx  of  presynaptic  stimuli.  [From  Eccles  (64).] 


158 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


lOmV    T 


wUWWwWUUUWwwUUWUUUvi 


1 1   I 


! 


FIG.  lo.  Depolarizing  electrogenesis  of  crayfish  mechanoreceptor  sense  organ  and  the  effects  it 
evokes  in  the  electrically  excitable  portion  of  the  cell.  Top:  A  weak  stretch  stimulus  (I)  caused  a 
depolarization  of  about  7  mv  across  the  membrane  of  the  cell  body.  This  was  maintained  until  the 
stretch  was  released  ( 1 ).  Aliddle:  Records  at  lower  amplification.  A  weak  stimulus  produced  a  low 
frequency  discharge  of  spikes.  Increased  stretch  (second  arrow)  caused  a  higher  frequency  discharge 
which  continued  with  some  slowing  as  long  as  the  stimulus  was  applied.  The  spikes  generated  during 
the  depolarization  develop  a  hyperpolarizing  undershoot  which  is  absent  when  the  response  is 
evoked  by  a  single  electrical  stimulus.  Bottom:  Three  increasingly  larger  stimulations  are  shown  in  A 
to  C.  The  spikes  produced  at  high  frequency  by  the  strongest  stimulus  (C)  were  diminished  in  ampli- 
tude and  at  the  end  were  no  longer  evoked,  while  the  receptor  continued  to  respond  with  its  sus- 
tained, summated  depolarization.  D  to  F:  The  return  of  responsiveness  of  the  electrically  excitable 
membrane  after  its  inactivation.  Note  that  the  average  level  of  the  depolarization  produced  by  the 
mechanoreceptor  dendrites  is  graded  with  the  degree  of  the  stimulus.  [From  Eyzaguirre  &  KufHer 
(66).] 


b)  P0STSVN.\PTIC  POTENTI.ALS  DURING  HYPERPOL.^RIZ.^- 

TiON  AND  DEPOLARIZATION.  P.s.p.'s  Can  bc  produccd 
during  hyperpolarization  of  the  cell,  while  spike 
electrogenesis  may  be  blocked  (fig.  1 2).  These  differ- 
ent effects  may  be  ascribed  directly  to  the  different 
modes  of  excitation  of  the  electrogenic  membrane 
components.  The  effects  produced  by  depolarization 
are  somewhat  more  complicated  but  can  also  be 
accounted  for  on  the  same  basis.  Superposition  of 
depolarization  by  a  brief  extrinsic  electrical  stimulus 
and  that  of  a  depolarizing  p.s.p.  enhances  the  excita- 
tion of  the  electrically  excitable  membrane  (4,  60, 
79).  The  spike  thus  arises  earlier  on  the  p.s.p.  since 
the  critical  level  of  depolarization  is  thereby  attained 
earlier. 


Sustained  depolarization,  in  some  cells  even  when 
rather  small,  blocks  spike  electrogenesis  (fig.  13} 
probably  (cf.  95,  96)  by  the  augmentation  of  sodium 
inactivation  and  potassium  conductance  that  it  causes 
in  electrically  excitable  membrane  (113).  Electrical 
inexcitaijility  of  synaptic  transducer  action  permits 
the  continued  development  of  p.s.p.'s  after  the  spike 
can  no  longer  be  produced  by  direct  or  neural  stimuli 
(figs.  II  and  13).  Other  manifestations  of  synaptic 
activity  can  also  be  evoked  when  the  spike  generating 
membrane  is  inactivated  by  ionically  induced  depolar- 
ization (50).  The  generator  potential  of  a  sense  organ 
(fig.  10)  may  also  continue  to  be  produced  even 
though  that  sustained  depolarization  inactivates  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane  and  no  spikes  can 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


159 


FIG.  II.  Desensitization  of  the  synaptic  membrane  of  frog  sartorius  muscle  fibers  by  sustained 
applications  of  acetylcholine.  The  drug  was  applied  through  each  of  two  pipettes  close  to  the  surface 
of  the  endplate.  From  one  pipette  it  was  released  at  regular  intervals  in  brief  jets  of  approximately 
constant  quantity.  These  testing  stimuli  are  signaled  by  dots  on  the  lower  line  in  each  set.  The  upper 
line  shows  the  response  of  the  endplate  recorded  with  an  internal  microelectrode.  The  e.p.p.'s  in 
these  records  are  compressed  on  the  slow  time  scale.  In  the  course  of  the  recordings  a  larger  longer- 
lasting  jet  of  diflTerent  amounts  of  acetylcholine  was  also  applied  to  the  endplate  as  a  conditioning 
stimulus.  Lejt:  An  otherwise  normal  preparation,  a:  The  conditioning  stimulus  was  a  weak  dose  of 
acetylcholine  applied  for  a  long  time,  b  to  d:  The  concentration  was  higher,  and  the  drug  was  applied 
for  different  times.  The  testing  responses  diminished  progressively  during  the  depolarization  pro- 
duced by  the  conditioning  stimulus.  Their  amplitudes  recovered  gradually  after  the  conditioning 
depolarization  had  ended.  Note  that  the  recovery  from  desensitization  is  not  associated  with  further 
change  in  potential.  The  recovery  process  therefore  is  not  controlled  by  the  membrane  potential. 
Right:  The  muscle  was  immersed  in  isotonic  potassium  sulfate  which  depolarized  the  fibers  and 
rendered  them  unresponsive  to  electrical  stimuli.  The  tested  muscle  fiber  was  made  inside-positive 
by  about  15  mv  by  means  of  an  intracellularly  applied  current.  The  synaptic  membrane  remains 
excitable  to  acetylcholine  following  these  procedures,  but  the  sign  of  the  response  is  now  reversed 
for  reasons  that  will  be  discussed  in  the  third  subsection  of  this  portion  of  this  chapter.  The  membrane 
still  exhibits  desensitization  to  different  intensities  of  the  excitant  drug  (jop  to  bollom).  The  desensiti- 
zation process  itself  therefore  is  also  not  controlled  by  the  membrane  potential.  At  the  end  of  the 
lower  record  the  internal  recording  electrode  was  withdrawn  from  the  muscle  fiber  (at  the  arrow},  the 
trace  going  from  a  level  of  internal  positivity  to  that  of  the  reference  zero  potential.  [From  Katz  & 
Theslefr(i27).] 


develop.  Thus,  the  sustained  depolarization  at 
sensory  receptor  terminals  or  at  synaptic  junctions, 
which  is  a  property  of  electrically  inexcitable  mem- 
brane while  initially  excitatory  for  the  associated 
electrically  excitable  spike  generator  can,  secondarily, 
inactivate  the  latter  and  thereby  block  further  conduc- 
tile  or  transmissional  activity. 

This  effect  accounts  for  Wedensky  inhibition,  the 
failure  of  transmission  produced  by  stimulating  the 
presynaptic  nerve  at  high  frequency.  Summated  and 


sustained  by  this  synaptic  drive,  the  depolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  at  first  evoke  a  few  spikes  which  then  cease  to 
develop  while  the  large  p.s.p.'s  continue  to  be  pro- 
duced by  the  afferent  stiinulation  (fig.  9).  Although 
Weden.sky  inhibition  is  probably  of  little  importance 
in  physiological  activity  of  organisms,  the  phenom- 
enon has  long  interested  physiologists  because  the 
attempt  to  explain  it  in  terms  of  electrical  excitability 
has  proved  uncon\incing  (cf  81,  141).  The  presence 
of  electrically  excitable  and  inexcitable  electrogenesis 


i6o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  12.  Different  effects  on  spikes  and  p.s.p.'s  of  cat  motoneurons  produced  with  different  amounts 
of  membrane  polarization.  Tlie  membrane  potential  was  changed  by  passing  an  appropriate  cur- 
rent through  the  recording  microclectrode.  A  Wheatstone  bridge  arrangement  balanced  out  the 
artifacts  caused  by  this  current,  but  as  a  consequence  absolute  levels  of  the  membrane  potential 
could  not  be  measured.  Upper  set  (^A  to  G):  Two  traces  are  simultaneously  recorded,  the  upper  indi- 
cating the  amount  of  current  flow  through  the  electrode,  the  lower  showing  the  recorded  potentials. 
.-1  to  C,  decreasing  amounts  of  depolarizing  current;  D,  no  applied  current;  E  to  G,  increasing 
amounts  of  hyperpolarizing  current.  The  records  are  aligned  so  that  the  peaks  of  the  spikes  coincide 
(upper  broken  line).  The  parallel  lower  broken  line  passes  through  the  point  at  which  the  spike 
begins.  When  the  strong  depolarizing  current  was  applied  in  .4,  it  quickly  evoked  a  direct  spike. 
A  subsequent  orthodromic  volley  evoked  a  p.s.p.  which  reached  the  critical  firing  level  but  found 
the  electrically  excitable  membrane  still  refractory.  Hence,  an  orthodromically  evoked  spike  was 
absent.  At  the  end  of  this  and  subsequent  records  is  a  50  mv  calibrating  pulse.  B,  C,  the  depolariza- 
tions from  the  applied  current  were  smaller.  They  did  not  elicit  a  spike;  but  summing  with  the  de- 
polarization of  the  p.s.p.  evoked  a  spike  earlier  than  the  orthodromic  volley  alone  did  (D).  Hyper- 
polarization  of  the  membrane  operated  in  the  opposite  direction,  hindering  the  orthodromically 
evoked  spike  which  appeared  markedly  late  on  the  p.s.p.  in  F,  and  was  absent  in  6',  although  the 
p.s.p.  in  the  hyperpolarized  neuron  was  larger  (compare  the  p.s.p.'s  in  A  and  G).  A  small  deflection 
which  follows  the  artifact  of  the  stimulus  to  the  nerve  and  precedes  the  p.s.p.  by  neaily  i  msec,  is 
probably  elcctrotonic  pick-up  of  activity  from  the  presynaptic  impulses.  Note  that  it  is  too  small  to 
evoke  the  spike.  Lower  set  QA'  to  F').  In  this  experiment  the  spikes  were  elicited  by  antidromic  in- 
vasion from  the  motor  axons.  A'  to  C,  decreasing  amounts  of  membrane  depolarization;  D\  no 
applied  current;  E'  and  F',  currents  applied  so  as  to  produce  increasing  membrane  hyperpolariza- 
tion.  The  antidromic  spike  (Z)')  shows  an  inflection  which  probably  represents  a  response  first  in 
the  axon  hillock  portion,  succeeded  by  involvement  of  the  rest  of  the  cell.  Depolarization  of  the 
cell  body  facilitates  its  invasion  by  the  antidromic  spike  and  minimizes  the  inflection  on  the  rising 
phase.  It  is  almost  absent  when  the  cell  is  strongly  depolarized  (.-l')-  Hyperpolarization  hinders  the 
invasion  of  the  cell  body  (F')  and  when  it  is  strong  (F')  prevents  the  response  of  the  soma.  The 
smaller,  early  component  is  then  seen  in  isolation  as  pick-up  at  the  cell  body  of  the  response  in  the 
axon  hillock  and  nerve  fiber.  Timing  pulses  at  i  msec,  intervals  are  injected  into  the  records.  [From 
Frank  &  Fuortes  (79).] 


in  the  same  cell  also  permits  blockade  of  spikes  by 
synaptic  depolarization  induced  by  drugs  that  excite 
the  synaptic  membrane  (fig.  13).  This  blockade  is 
frequently  useful  clinically  but  it  in  often  misnamed 
as'curarization"  (cf.  96).  Blockade  by  o'-tubocurarine 
and  other  similarly  acting  agents  operates  through  an 


entirely   different    mechanism   as   will    be   described 
below. 

c)    ELECTROCHEMICAL    GRADATION    AND    REVERSAL    OF 

POSTSYNAPTIC  POTENTIALS.  Although  Synaptic  trans- 
ducer action  is  not  responsive  to  electrical  stimuli, 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


l6l 


o 

D" 

0",; 

■^ 

— - — 

E" 

E"' 

•tz 


FIG.  13.  Differential  effects  of  depolarization  on  the  spike 
and  p.s.p.  of  the  eel  electroplaque.  Column  A  to  F,  direct 
stimulation i  columns  A'  to  F',  etc.,  weak,  moderately  strong, 
and  very  strong  stimuli  to  a  nerve.  A  to  A'",  the  response  of  the 
normal  cell.  The  resting  potential  is  about  80  mv  seen  as  the 
deflection  of  the  active  trace  downward  from  the  zero  line 
(upper  trace).  The  strong  direct  stimulus  evoked  a  spike  with 
very  brief  latency  (.4).  The  weak  neural  volley  caused  a  p.s.p. 
CA'"),  the  stronger  also  a  spike  (.-!"  and  .-!'")  arising  out  of  the 
p.s.p.  The  cell  was  treated  with  weak  physostigmine  (25  ^g  per 
ml  solution)  for  78  min.,  and  weak  acetylcholine  (i  ixg  per  mg) 
for  the  last  58  min.  of  that  period.  These  drugs  had  no  effect 
on  the  potentials;  5  ftg  per  ml  acetylcholine  were  then  added. 
Depolarization  developed,  the  spikes  36  min.  later  becoming 
smaller,  but  the  p.s.p.  was  unaffected  (S  to  /?'").  The  diminish- 
ing electrically  evoked  response  g  min.  later  (C  to  C")  became 
graded,  as  seen  by  its  larger  size  in  response  to  the  strong  neural 
volley.  These  effects  progressed  during  the  next  ig  min.  (/)  to 
Z)'")  and  1 1  min.  thereafter  (£  to  £"").  The  p.s.p.  to  the 
threshold  neural  volley  decreased  (£").  but  was  still  evident 
later  (F')  when  the  electiically  excitable  membrane  no  longer 
responded  to  a  much  stronger  direct  stimulus  (F).  The  p.s.p. 
to  a  maximal  neural  stimulation  (f ")  was  still  about  as  large 
as  initially  (^"')-  This  p.s.p.  was  capable  of  evoking  a  small 
graded  response  of  the  electrically  excitable  membi^ane,  as  seen 
by  the  delayed  additional  potential  on  the  falling  phase.  [From 
Altamirano  el  al.  (6).] 

the  magnitudes  of  the  p.s.p.'s  and  also  their  signs  may 
be  affected  by  changes  in  the  membrane  potential 
(52,  60,  97).  These  effects,  however,  are  secondary 
and,  indeed,  are  explicable  only  by  the  electrical 
inexcitability  of  postsynaptic  electrogenic  membrane. 
Suppose  that  a  transducer  action  increases  solely 
the  permittivity  for  CI~.  More  of  this  ion  being  present 
in  the  external  fluid,  it  tends  to  flow  inward  until  the 
increased  internal  negativity  tends  to  prevent  further 
entry.  Thus,  the  direction  and  amount  of  ionic  flow 
is  determined    both  by   the   chemical   concentration 


gradient  and  by  the  electrical  potential  gradient,  the 
coinbination  being  termed  the  electrochemical  gradi- 
ent. For  a  given  concentration  gradient  there  is  a 
corresponding  potential  gradient  at  which  the  flow 
of  ions  is  balanced  by  the  opposite  force  of  the  elec- 
trical charge.  If  the  membrane  resting  potential  is 
increased  by  some  means,  the  electrogenesis  caused  by 
influx  of  Cl^  would  reach  the  electrochemical  poten- 
tial (Ecr)  for  that  ion  sooner.  The  hyperpolarizing 
p.s.p.  would  therefore  appear  to  be  smaller.  If  the 
membrane  potential  is  made  more  negative  than  Eci~, 
Cl~  in  the  cell  would  be  forced  outward.  The  p.s.p. 
would  then  appear  to  reverse  in  sign,  depolarizing  the 
hyperpolarized  membrane  approximately  to  the  level 
of  Eci~.  This  effect  is  seen  in  figure  14I. 

The  p.s.p.  can  likewise  be  affected  by  changing  the 
Cl^  concentration  either  of  the  interior  or  of  the 
exterior.  For  example,  suppose  that  the  external  Cl~ 
is  replaced  by  another  anion  which  does  not  penetrate 
the  membrane.  During  transducer  action,  Cl~  would 
move  out  from  the  cell  since  it  is  now  more  concen- 
trated in  the  interior.  The  electrogenesis  of  hyper- 
polarizing p.s.p.'s  can  thus  be  reversed  to  depolariza- 
tion. The  effect  of  increasing  internal  Cl~  is  seen  in 
figure  14.  Secondary  electrochemical  effects  therefore 
can  change  the  amplitude  or  sign  of  the  p.s.p. 

In  the  case  of  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s,  increase  of 
resting  membrane  potential  inay  lead  to  increased 
electrical  responses;  decrease  of  the  resting  potential 
decreases  and  eventually  reverses  the  sign  of  the  de- 
polarizing p.s.p.'s.  These  various  conditions  for 
electrochemical  grading  and  reversal  of  the  p.s.p.'s 
are  found  experimentally  (figs.  4,  11,  14).  The  grading 
and  re\ersal  of  p.s.p.'s  are  strong  evidence  that  the 
tran.sducer  actions  of  synaptic  membrane  are  not 
electrically  excitable  (97)  since  the  physiological 
responses  are  not  affected  even  by  violent  changes  of 
the  membrane  potential,  though  the  electrogenesis 
itself  is  modified. 

Cat  motoneuron  p.s.p.'s  are  electrochemically 
reversible  (cf  60),  but  anomalies  have  been  observed 
that  are  instructive.  In  theory,  as  outlined  above,  the 
apparent 'depolarization'  of  a  reversed  hyperpolariz- 
ing p.s.p.  should  only  return  the  membrane  potential 
to  the  saine  level  as  does  the  hyperpolarization  of  the 
normal  p.s.p.  The  'depolarization'  therefore  should 
not  reach  the  critical  firing  level  for  the  spike,  the 
membrane  in  theory  still  remaining  at  a  hyperpolar- 
ized level,  and  the  'depolarizing'  p.s.p.  should  not 
become  excitatory.  Frequently,  however,  this  is  not 
the  case  when  the  reversal  is  produced  by  changing 
the  ionic  concentration  gradients  of  the  motoneuron. 


j62 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


IT 


CX  "   B  I  C 


FIG.  14.  Reversals  of  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s.  Intracellular 
recording  from  biceps-semitendinosus  motoneuron  of  cat;  hy- 
perpolarizing p.s.p.'s  evoked  by  stimulating  quadriceps  nerve. 
I.  A  to  G:  The  resting  potential  was  —74  mv  (Z)).  Depolari- 
zation augmented  the  p.s.p.  (.4  to  C).  Hyperpolarization  at 
first  diminished  the  p.s.p.,  the  equilibrium  potential  for  ionic 
movements  without  electrogenesis  being  at  —82  mv  (£).  Fur- 
ther hyperpolarization  reversed  the  sign  of  the  p.s.p.  (F,  G). 
The  Cl~  content  of  the  motoneuron  was  then  increased  and 
K+  decreased  (H  to  Z,).  Immediately  thereafter  (J  to  Z.)  the 
p.s.p.  was  'depolarizing'  at  all  but  the  least  negative  values 
'(//,  /)  of  the  membrane  potential.  M  to  Q:  Recovery  toward 
initial  condition  not  yet  complete  3  to  4  min.  later.  II.  Reversal 
of  the  sign  of  the  p.s.p.  was  produced  by  changing  the  ionic 
gradient  of  Cl~.  Initial  response  0-1)  was  altered  in  B  and  C  by 
increeising  intracellular  Cl~  as  a  result  of  diffusion  out  of  the 
tip  of  the  microelectrode.  Depolarization  of  the  membrane  to 
—  27  mv  by  an  applied  current  restored  the  sign  of  the  p.s.p. 
(D).  The  Cl~  gradient  was  then  changed  drastically.  The  re- 
versals of  the  p.s.p.'s  produced  soon  thereafter  (is  to  G)  oc- 
curred without  significant  change  of  the  resting  potential  and 
were  sufficient  to  excite  spikes,  at  first  with  brief  latency  (£), 
then  progressively  later  (F  and  G).  Each  record  is  formed  by 
superposition  of  many  traces.  In  G  it  is  seen  that  the  depolari- 


The  initial  resting  potential  may  then  be  altered  little 
or  not  at  all,  as  is  also  the  case  with  microinjections  of 
ions  into  squid  giant  axons  (cf.  91,  105).  The  changed 
chemical  gradient  of  the  motoneuron  then  causes  a 
reversal  of  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  into  depolarization 
which  develops  at,  or  near,  the  initial  resting  poten- 
tial. The  reversed  'inhibitory'  p.s.p.  now  may  elicit  a 
spike  (fig.  14II). 

In  crustacean  muscle  fibers  (68,  73)  and  stretch 
receptors  (130)  the  equilibrium  potential  for  the 
inhibitory  p.s.p.  is  nearly  identical  with  the  resting 
potential.  Stimulating  the  inhibitory  axon  therefore 
may  elicit  no  p.s.p.,  or  the  latter  may  be  small,  and 
of  either  sign.  Nevertheless,  the  membrane  potential 
tends  to  be  clamped  at  or  near  the  resting  potential, 
particularly  if  the  activity  of  the  inhibitory  synaptic 
membrane  increases  markedly  the  permittivity  of 
the  membrane  for  the  relevant  ions  (K+,  Cl~  or 
both).  Excitatory  depolarization,  elicited  at  the 
same  time,  by  p.s.p.'s  in  muscle  fibers  or  by  mechano- 
sensory  dendrites  in  stretch  receptors,  therefore  tends 
to  be  depressed.  When  the  inhibitory  synapses  of 
lobster  muscle  fibers  are  maximally  activated  by 
7-aminobutyric  acid  the  membrane  potential  is 
increased  by  about  4  mv,  but  the  membrane  con- 
ductance is  increased  about  8-fold  (Grundfest, 
Reuben  &  Rickles,  in  preparation;  cf.  99). 

d)  LATENCY  OF  posTSYN.-^PTic  POTENTIALS.  As  men- 
tioned above,  the  onset  of  the  explosive  response  of 
electrically  excitable  membrane  depends  upon  the 
attainment  of  a  critical  level  of  depolarization.  A 
strong  electrical  stimulus,  causing  rapid  depolariza- 
tion to  that  level,  therefore  evokes  a  spike  with  vanish- 
ingly  brief  latency  (fig.  6),  this  fact  having  been 
established   by  Bernstein  in    1871   (19).    In  all  cases 


zation  occasionally  fell  below  the  critical  firing  level  and  con- 
tinued to  decrease  in  the  later  records  (//  to  L).  III.  The 
membrane  generating  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  maintains  its 
pharmacological  individuality,  although  the  electrical  response 
may  be  reversed  and  is  then  indistinguishable  from  that  of  a 
depolarizing  p.s.p.  Prior  to  taking  this  scries  of  records  the 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  evoked  in  the  biceps-semitendinosus 
motoneuron  by  stimulating  quadriceps  afTercnts  was  reversed 
(by  diflfusing  Cl~  from  the  electrode  into  the  cell).  This  response 
is  shown  at  the  beginning  of  each  record  (/).  Following  it  is  a 
depolarizing  p.s.p.  (£)  evoked  by  stimulating  afferents  in  the 
biceps-semitendinosus  nerve.  Strychnine  salicylate  (o.  i  mg  per 
kg)  was  injected  after  record  A  and  caused  progressive  diminu- 
tion of  7,  but  no  change  in  £  during  the  next  4  lo-sec.  intervals 
(fi  to  £).  The  reversed  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  almost  disap- 
peared after  a  second  injection  (f ).  [From  Eccles  (60).] 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


163 


where  appropriate  data  are  available  (of.  97)  the 
neurally  evoked  response  arises  after  an  appreciable 
irreducible  latency  (fig.  6),  or  synaptic  delay  (44; 
cf.  140). 

Between  the  arrival  of  the  presynaptic  impulse  and 
the  onset  of  the  p.s.p.  of  cat  motoneurons  there  is  a 
latency  of  about  0.3  to  0.4  msec.  (59,  p.  130).  In  the 
eel  electroplaque  the  latency  attains  i  to  2.5  msec. 
(4).  This  delay  is  not  conducive  to,  nor  consistent  with, 
electrical  excitation  of  synaptic  membrane  by  the 
action  current  of  the  presynaptic  impulse  (97)  as  was 
pointed  out  by  du  Bois-Revmond  (55)  and  Bernstein 
(20). 

Presumably,  synaptic  latency  is  compounded  from 
the  durations  required:  (T)  for  release  of  transmitter 
from  the  presynaptic  terminals;  («)  for  its  transit 
across  a  synaptic  space  of  about  100  A  (52,  54,  152, 
153'  '74'  553)'  ^"^^  ("')  fo'"  development  of  the 
electrogenic  reactions  when  the  transmitter  acts 
upon  the  postsynaptic  membrane.  The  details  of 
none  of  these  components  are  as  yet  known. 

e)  electrotonic  effects  of  presynaptic  impulse 
UPON  postsynaptic  region.  Intracellular  recording 
revealed  (cf.  59,  60)  that  the  presynaptic  spike  not 
only  arrived  too  early,  but  also  that  its  electrotonic 
efTect  was  too  little  to  cause  electrical  excitation  of  the 
postsynaptic  membrane.  Indirect  stimulation  of  the 
eel  electroplaque  (fig.  6C,  D)  excites  the  terminal  fibers 
innervating  the  cell  membrane.  Their  spikes  must 
have  occurred  with  vanishingly  small  latency  upon 
strong  stimulation  (Z)).  However,  no  trace  of  elec- 
trotonic effects  in  the  electroplaque  was  found.  The 
presynaptic  impulses  could  not  be  observed  even  at 
high  sensitivity  of  recording  (fig.  3).  In  other  prepara- 
tions small,  brief  as  well  as  early  electrotonic  pick  up 
of  the  presynaptic  spikes  is  observed  (cf.  figs.  19, 
2  J  A).  The  magnitudes,  i  or  2  mv,  are  insignificant  for 
electrical  excitation  which  requires  critical  depolari- 
zations of  some  10  to  40  mv. 

Among  the  possibilities  for  accounting  for  the  small- 
ne.ss  of  electrotonic  effects  across  synapses  are  the 
following. 

/)  Theresistanceof  one  or  both  cell  membranes  may 
be  very  high.  In  most  types  of  synapses  the  presynap- 
tic terminals  making  contact  with  postsynaptic 
membrane  are  very  small  and  this  alone  would  de- 
crease the  electrotonic  effects.  However,  the  contact 
between  the  pre-  and  postfibers  in  the  .squid  giant 
axon  synapse  are  broad,  yet  the  electrotonic  post- 
junctional potential  is  small  (fig.  19).  Likewise,  in  the 
eel  electroplaque  where  the  innervation  is  diffused 


widely  over  the  cell  membrane  electrotonic  effects  are 
small. 

2)  The  bulk  of  the  synaptic  current  may  be  shunted 
by  the  subsynaptic  space. 

3)  If  the  nerve  terminals  were  themselves  elec- 
trically inexcitable  neurosecretory  regions  the  spike 
would  not  invade  the  nerve  proximate  to  the  synapse. 
The  extrinsic  current  in  the  synaptic  region  would 
thus  be  already  attenuated  by  electrotonic  losses. 

f)     CHEMICAL     SENSITIVITY     OF     SYNAPTIC     MEMBRANE. 

Many  varieties  of  drugs  exert  effects  upon  synapses, 
but  they  either  do  not  affect  electrically  excitable 
membrane  or  do  so  only  when  applied  in  high  con- 
centrations and  for  long  times  (6,  96).  The  high 
sensitivity  of  synaptic  membrane  to  chemicals  is  prob- 
ably also  a  con.sequence  of  its  chemical  excitability. 
Thus,  many  drugs  cause  synaptic  electrogenesis, 
thereby  mimicking  the  effects  of  the  natural  trans- 
mitter agents.  The.se  substances  are  known  as  '  de- 
polarizing drugs'  but  are  more  properly  designated 
as  'synapse  activators'  (95,  96)  for  their  action  is 
merely  that  of  excitants.  The  type  of  synaptic  electro- 
genesis  is  determined  by  the  nature  of  the  synapse 
itself  For  example,  acetylcholine  and  its  mimetics 
cause  depolarization  when  applied  to  muscle  end- 
plates  or  sympathetic  ganglia,  but  when  applied  to 
the  cardiac  pacemaker  synapses  which  are  hyper- 
polarized  by  vagal  stimuli  the  drugs  also  cause  hyper- 
polarization  (49,  1 20).  A  second  group  of  substances, 
the' synapse  inactivators',  hinder  or  prevent  excitation 
of  the  membrane  bv  the  activator  drugs.  These  are 
also  called  '  nondepolarizing  competitive   inhibitors' 

(155)- 

Both  types  of  substances  may  cause  block  of  trans- 
mission. Depolarizing  excitatory  p.s.p.'s  are  dimin- 
ished in  amplitude  or  prevented  by  the  inactivating 
drugs.  The  decrease  of  the  p.s.p.  below  the  critical 
level  for  discharging  spikes  is  the  mechanism  of  the 
synaptic  blockading  action  of  these  drugs.  Curare  or 
</-tubocurarine  act  in  this  way  (fig.  15).  A  general 
feature  of  blockade  by  inactivating  drugs  is  that  the 
electrically  excitaijle  membrane  is  affected  little  or 
not  at  all.  Thus,  the  postjunctional  cell  can  remain 
directly  excitable. 

Synapse-activating  drugs  induce  transmissional 
blockade  by  an  entirely  different  mechanism  which  is 
referrable  to  the  fundamentally  different  excitabilities 
of  electrogenic  membrane.  Acting  on  the  synaptic 
membrane,  the  drugs  evoke  depolarization  of  the 
excitatory  synapses.  This  electrogenesis,  sustained  in 


164 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  15.  Effect  of  nondepolarizing  synaptic  blocliing  agents 
on  the  responses  of  the  eel  electroplaque.  Direct  stimulation  of 
the  cell  is  represented  in  A  to  F,  neural  excitation  in  A'  to  C. 
The  initial  responses  to  both  stimuli  are  shown  in  .4  and  A'.  At 
3  min.  after  substituting  a  bathing  solution  containing  5  mg 
per  ml  o'-tubocurarinc,  the  directly  elicited  spike  was  unchanged 
(B),  but  synaptic  excitation  was  less  effective,  the  spike  arising 
later  on  the  smaller  p.s.p.  (£')■  A*  5  ""in.  (C,  C)  the  directly 
elicited  response  was  still  unaffected,  but  the  p.s.p.  had  de- 
creased so  much  (,C")  that  it  was  seen  only  with  repetitive 
stimulation  at  50  per  sec,  and  produced  a  single  small  'spike,' 
after  which  it  could  no  longer  affect  the  electrically  excitable 
membrane.  The  latter,  however,  remained  fully  responsise  to  a 
direct  stimulus  41  min.  later  (/)),  but  eventually  this  responsive- 
ness decreased  (96  min.  later,  E;  and  1 1  o  min.  after  this,  F). 
The  resting  potential  of  the  cell  was  unchanged.  Calibration 
100  mv  and  msec.  [From  Altamirano  et  al.  (6).] 


the  presence  of  the  chemical  stunulant,  leads  to 
inactivation  of  the  spike-generating  membrane  as 
described  above.  The  entire  cell  may  then  become 
inexcitable  by  direct  stimuli  (fig.  13).  In  the  case  of 
skeletal  muscle  fibers,  the  inactivating  depolarization 
is  confined  to  the  regions  of  the  endplates  and  neuro- 
muscular transmi.ssion  is  blocked  Ijecause  these 
regions  do  not  generate  spikes.  Neuromuscular  jjlock- 
ade  evoked  by 'depolarizing'  synap.se  activating  drugs, 
and  blockade  also  at  neuronal  synapses,  are  usually 
preceded  by  a  brief  period  of  hyperactivity.  The  dis- 
organized contraction  of  muscles,  frequently  but 
incorrectly  termed  'fasciculation',  is  due  to  the  initial 
excitatory  effect  of  the  synaptic  depolarization,  the 
individual  muscle  fibers  responding  to  this  stimulus 
before  their  spikes  are  inactivated.  Blockade  by  the 
truly  curarizing  drugs,  the  inacti\ators  of  synaptic 
activity,  is  not  preceded  l)y  the  excitatory  eflfects. 


Postsynaptic  Potential':  as  \onpropagated 
'  Standing'  Potentials 

The  local  circuit  current  of  activity,  in  combination 
with  electrical  excitability,  makes  possible  the  con- 
ductile  property  of  electrically  excitable,  regenera- 
tively  responsive  membrane  (fig.  8).  The  all-or-none 
character  of  the  spike  then  leads  to  decreinentless 
propagation.  A  consequence  of  electrical  inexcitaljil- 
itv  is  that  the  p.s.p.'s  do  not  set  off  activity  in  other 
portions  of  synaptic  membrane.  The  electrogenesis  is 
therefore  localized  and  does  not  propagate  except 
electrotonically  as  mentioned  earlier  (fig.  2).  This 
'standing'  nature  of  p.s.p.'s  has  important  physiologi- 
cal consequences  that  will  be  discus.sed  later.  It  also 
introduces  a  technical  complication  in  the  interpre- 
tation of  potentials  recorded  from  volume  conductors. 
The  rules  that  apply  to  potentials  generated  by  a 
travelling  impulse  (cf.  140,  141)  need  not  hold, 
particularly  since  hyperpolarizing  as  well  as  depolar- 
izing p.s.p.'s  of  the '  standing'  variety  can  be  produced 
at  various  sites  (cf.  161-167).'  It  is  of  more  than  his- 
torical interest  to  note  that  Sherrington  and  his 
colleagues  sue:gested  that  the  central  excitatory  state 
(c.e.s.)  "is  a  specialized  manifestation  of  local  exci- 
tatory state."  (44,  p.  43).  In  the  present  da\-  contexts, 
the  central  excitatory  state  may  be  identified  in  large 
measure  with  occurrence  of  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s, 
and  the  central  inhibitory  state  with  of  hyperpolariz- 
ing p.s.p.'s.  However,  phenomena  such  as  desensitiza- 
tion  (p.  157)  may  obscure  or  eliminate  this  parallelism 
between  potentials  and  excitability.  Thus,  as  appears 
in  figure  11,  the  depolarized  but  also  desensitized 
endplate  may  not  respond  to  a  stimulus.  Such  a  condi- 
tion might  lead  to  blockade  of  transmission  although 
the  background  is  one  of  depolarization.  Desensitiza- 
tion  of  hyperpolarizing  synapses  has  not  yet  been 
described,  l)ut  its  occurrence  is  not  unlikely.  If  it 
exists,  it  could  provide  cases  of  lifting  of  inhibitory 
blockade  in  the  face  of  a  background  of  hyperpolariza- 
tion.  It  will  be  shown  later  that  the  responsiveness  of 
electrically  excitable  membrane  (its  local  excitatory 
state)  can  change  without  a  parallel  change  of  the 
membrane  potential,  although  the  excitability  of  this 
meinbrane  is  also  a  reflection  of  the  action  of  tjraded 
local  responses. 

'  An  extreme  example  of  localized  activity  which  is  therefore 
highly  instructive  has  been  reported  in  the  cat  cortex  (150,  fig. 
19).  Within  a  range  of  20  /i  in  the  depth  of  the  cerebral  cortex 
the  pattern  and  degree  of  electrical  acti%ity  undergoes  great 
modifications. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


165 


Interaction  of  Graded  Responses 

Generated  and  propagated  in  electrically  inexcit- 
able  membrane,  p.s.p.'s  can  spread  only  by  electro- 
tonus  (fig.  2),  passively,  without  evokina;  new  activity 
and  with  considerable  decrement.  As  weak  depolariza- 
tions, p.s.p.'s  acting  upon  adjacent,  electrically  excit- 
able membrane  may  evoke  graded  local  responses 
(figs.  3,  7,  13).  The  latter  are  also  decrementally 
propagated,  but  the  decrement  may  be  smaller  than 
in  the  case  of  p.s.p.'s.  The  depolarizing  activity  of  a 
graded  local  response  at  one  site  may,  in  turn,  give 
rise  to  some  degree  of  active  response  at  other  sites. 
Thus,  depending  upon  the  local  excitability  of  the 
membrane  and  the  amount  of  initial  local  response, 
this  graded  depolarization  may  spread  only  passively, 
or  it  may  propagate  with  various  degrees  of  active 
contribution.  The  ultimate  extent  of  the  latter  is  that 
which  evokes  a  spike.  This  explosive  process  domi- 
nates subsequent  events  since  the  magnitude  of  its 
electrical  activity  usually  far  exceeds  the  require- 
ments for  continued  local  circuit  electrical  excitation. 
In  other  words,  when  the  spike  generator  has  a  high 
safety  factor,  decrementlcss  propagation  is  the  rule. 

The  nature  of  graded  local  responses  of  electrically 
excitable  membrane  will  be  discussed  below  (p.  167) 
in  conjunction  with  the  mechanisms  of  gradation  of 
p.s.p.'s.  Here,  it  is  desired  to  stress  that  the  two 
graded  responses  provide  a  pathway  for  summative 
gradation  as  a  transition  to  the  all-or-none  spike 
(fig-  3)- 


EVENTS  IN  SYNAPTIC  TRANSMISSIO.V 

Functional  Interrelations  Within  Single  Cell 

A  generalized  schema  of  the  activities  within  a 
single  unit  in  a  transmission  chain  is  shown  in  figure 
16.  The  input  of  the  cell,  the  synaptic  surface  in  the 
present  context,  but  which  may  also  be  the  receptor 
surface  of  a  sensory  cell  (cf.  94,  96,  97),  is  activated 
by  a  specific  chemical  stimulus  and  develops  an 
electrical  response.  Only  the  depolarizing  variety, 
excitatory  for  the  conductile  mechanism,  need  be 
considered  now.  The  p.s.p.  may  ije  brief  or  long  and 
may  give  rise  to  a  single  spike  or  to  a  train  of  impulses. 
This  conductile  activity,  arriving  at  the  terminus  of 
the  cell,  causes  secretory  activity  which  releases  a 
transmitter  agent  that  can  excite  another  unit  of  the 
transinission  chain  or  an  effector. 


INPUT     ,    CONDUCTILE    |  OUTPUT 


GENERATOR 
ACTIVITY 


CONDUCTILE 
ACTIVITY 


TERMINAL 
ELECTROGENESIS 


FIG.  16.  Diagrammatic  representation  of  functional  com- 
ponents and  electrical  responses  of  a  receptor  cell  or  neuron. 
The  electrically  inexcitable  input  produces  electrogenesis 
graded  in  proportion  to  its  specific  stimulus  and  sustained  as 
long  as  the  latter  is  applied.  The  possibility  of  hypcrpolarizing 
electrogenesis  is  shown  but  is  not  further  considered.  The  de- 
polarization at  the  input,  operating  upon  the  conductile  elec- 
trically excitable  component,  can  evoke  spikes  in  the  latter 
coded  in  number  and  frequency  in  proportion  to  the  depolari- 
zation. These  signals,  propagated  to  the  output,  there  command 
secretory  activity,  roughly  proportional  to  the  information  en- 
coded in  their  message  and  sustained  as  long  as  the  message 
demands.  The  transmitter  released  at  the  output  can  initiate  a 
synaptic  transfer  by  operating  upon  the  depolarizing  input  of 
another  cell.  The  possibility  of  a  special  output  electrogenesis 
is  indicated  but  is  not  further  considered.  The  lower  electrical 
portion  of  this  diagram  may  be  compared  with  records  from  a 
sense  organ  (fig.  10).  [From  Grundfest  (97).] 


Evolution  of  Electrogenic  Membrane 

The  occurrence  of  receptor-effector  cells  in  primi- 
tive metazoa  suggested  to  Parker  (154)  that  the  nerv- 
ous systein  ev'olved  by  parcellation  of  the  two  func- 
tions among  separate  receptor  and  effector  cells  with 
the  interposition  of  a  conductile  element  extending 
from  the  receptor  cell.  Later  in  evolution,  correlational 
neuronal  cells  were  presumed  to  have  arisen.  This 
evolutionary  schema  may  also  be  applied  to  the 
individual  cells,  neurons  and  muscle  fibers  as  well  as 
receptors  (103).  The  receptor  portion  of  the  priinitive 
unit  was  probably  sensitive  to  specific  stimuli  and  this 
characteristic  is  retained  at  the  electrically  inexcitable 
input  of  the  present  nerve  cell,  mu.scle  fiber,  gland  or 
receptor  (fig.  16).  The  ouptut  likewise  inay  be  con- 
sidered as  representing  the  primitive  effector,  frankly 
so  in  the  contractile  muscle  fibers  or  in  glands.  The 
terminals  of  the  neurons  likewise  probably  embody 
the  secretory  capacity  of  primitive  units  adapted  to  a 
new  function,  transmission  at  close  contact.  Other 
neurosecretory  cells  of  more  general  function  are  also 


1 66 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


common  (179)  and  the  electrically  inexcitable  secre- 
tory cells  of  the  adrenal  medulla  are  regarded  as 
second  order  autonomic  neurons  (cf.  i  77). 

The  conductile  portion  of  the  neuron,  generating 
all-or-none  spikes  and  therefore  capable  of  decre- 
mentless  propagation,  requires  electrical  excitability 
for  this  function.  It  is  probably  a  later  evolutionary 
development  (21)  brought  about  in  the  course  of 
extension  of  the  cells  in  the  metazoa  and  of  their 
participation  in  complexly  organized  activity.  That 
the  conductile  activity  represents  a  new  evolutionary 
step,  mediated  by  a  structure  interposed  between  the 
primitive  input  and  output  sections,  is  also  suggested 
by  the  absence  of  conductile  electrogenesis  in  gland 
cells  and  by  their  electrical  inexcitability  (96,  97). 
The  occurrence  of  muscle  fibers  which  are  also  not 
electrically  excitable  and  which  generate  no  spikes 
(4,  34,  35,  97)  reinforces  this  view.  Classifying  distinc- 
tions with  respect  to  excitability  and  the  types  of 
responses  of  electrogenic  membranes  are  by  no  means 
exhaustive  of  the  different  varieties.  Pharmacological 
differences  of  various  kinds  specify  an  even  greater 
diversity  amongst  excitable,  electrogenic  membranes. 
These  differences  are  not  to  be  seen  by  anatomical 
methods,  nor  indeed,  by  electrophysiological  means 
alone,  since  pharmacologically  distinct  varieties  of 
membrane  can  all  generate  similar  types  of  electrical 
responses  (fig.  14III). 

Transmitter  Actions 

The  varieties  of  transmitters  will  be  treated  below; 
the  present  discussion  will  be  confined  to  the  general 
electrophysiological  aspects.  From  this  point  of  view, 
the  precise  chemical  natures  of  the  substances  are  of 
little  moment,  the  important  feature  being  that  they 
all  activate  synaptic  electrogenesis.  It  is  unlikely  that 
the  sign  of  the  p.s.p.  is  affected  by  the  excitant  agent. 
Thus,  as  noted  above,  acetylcholine  is  a 'depolarizing' 
substance  for  excitatory  p.s.p.'s  but  activating  inhibi- 
tory synapses,  as  in  the  pacemaker  of  the  heart  it  is  a 
"  hyperpolarizing'  agent.  The  characteristics  of  the 
transmitters  will,  however,  determine  to  .some  extent 
the  character  of  the  p.s.p.  a)  For  example,  if  the 
transmitter  is  a  large  complex  molecule,  it  is  unlikely 
that  it  would  be  available  in  large  concentrations  at 
the  terminals  of  the  presynaptic  element.  The  amount 
of  total  excitant  might  therefore  be  limited  in  propor- 
tion to  the  quantity  secreted  during  a  single  activity. 
Thus,  a  single  afferent  volley  might  cause  a  number 
of  p.s.p.'s,  but  repetitive  activity  might  rapidly  ex- 
haust the  available  transmitter,  b)  Molecular  dimen- 


sions and  configurations  might  also  determine  the 
rapidity  of  diffusion  of  the  transmitter  from  its  site  of 
release  to  its  site  of  action.  The  distances  involved, 

o 

although  probably  only  about  100  A  are  significant 
in  terms  of  molecules,  c)  The  kinetics  of  interaction 
between  the  transmitter  and  the  postsynaptic  electro- 
genic surface  may  also  be  in  part  determined  by  the 
transmitter  itself.  For  example,  it  is  conceivable  that 
two  different  agents  might  act  on  similar  synaptic 
sites  with  different  kinetics,  giving  rise  to  differences 
in  the  p.s.p.'s  evoked  by  each.  Studies  in  kinetics  of 
these  interactions  are  only  now  beginning  (cf  53,  127) 
and  the  nature  of  interaction  is  as  yet  unknown. 
Analogy  with  other  processes  is  usually  invoked  and 
two  models  which  are  at  present  fashionable,  actisa- 
tion  processes  of  enzyme  reactions  and  antigen- 
antibody  combinations,  are  not  necessarily  mutually 
exclusive.  The  transmitter  agent  is  presumed  to  com- 
bine with  some  '  receptor'  sites  of  the  synaptic  mem- 
brane (cf  2,  9,  14).  (f)  The  chemical  properties  of  the 
transmitter  may  also  determine  the  characteristics  of 
the  p.s.p.  Thus,  a  labile  agent  such  as  acetylcholine 
may  be  rapidly  destroyed,  and  it  might  give  rise  to 
shorter  p.s.p.'s  than  would  a  more  stable  excitant  of 
the  same  synaptic  site  (cf.  53).  0  Likewi.se,  the  degree 
of  chemical  binding  between  the  transmitter  and  the 
'  receptor'  or  the  stability  of  the  complex  may  play 
similar  roles  in  determining  the  duration  of  the  p.s.p., 
or  in  its 'competitive'  behavior  toward  an  inactivating 
synaptic  drug.  /)  Although  a  transmitter  agent  may 
activate  a  given  type  of  receptor  it  may  also  be  an 
inactivator  of  other  types.  Thus,  the  transmitter  at 
inhibitory  synapses  of  some  invertebrate  muscle  fibers 
is  thought  to  be  an  inactivator  of  the  excitatory  syn- 
apses (68,  73).  g~)  A  given  synaptic  complex  might  be 
composed  of  several  \arieties  of  receptors,  although 
all  generating  the  same  kind  of  p.s.p.  Yet,  one  trans- 
mitter might  inactivate  some  of  the  receptors  while 
another  transmitter  did  not,  and  the  p.s.p.'s  would 
vary  accordingly. 

Two  of  the  factors,  the  transit  time  of  the  trans- 
mitter across  the  synaptic  gap  (6  in  the  preceding) 
and  an  induction  period  (c  above),  probably  deter- 
mine the  synaptic  latency  as  noted  earlier.  Together 
these  two  processes  may  last  several  milliseconds. 

Genesis  nf  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Important  information  on  this  matter  derives  from 
the  occurrence  of  spontaneous  'miniature'  p.s.p.'s  at 
muscle  endplates.  Probably  this  activity  is  generated 
bv  random  releases  of  transmitter  from  presynaptic 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


167 


sites  (52).  The  miniature  p.s.p.'s  are  probably  quanta! 
in  the  sense  that  each  is  composed  of  a  minimum 
electrical  change  generated  by  a  'packet'  of  trans- 
mitter agent.  The  random  release  of  packets  from 
presynaptic  terminals  at  different  synaptic  sites  and 
the  electrical  inexcitability  of  the  postsynaptic  mem- 
brane combine  to  cause  local  miniature  p.s.p.'s  gener- 
ated now  at  one  site,  now  at  another  (51). 

Depolarization  of  the  presynaptic  nerve  terminals 
augments  the  frequency  of  miniature  e.p.p.'s  in  frog 
muscle  fibers  (52).  Similar  data  (137)  on  rat  dia- 
phragm muscle  are  even  more  decisive  (fig.  1 7). 
Depolarizing  electrotonus  applied  to  the  phrenic 
nerve  increases  the  rate  of  the  miniature  activity  very 
markedly,  while  hyperpolarizing  the  nerve  terminals 
decreases  the  activity.  E.xcess  magnesium,  which  de- 
presses the  release  of  transmitter  agents  (cf.  52), 
depresses  or  eliminates  the  effects  of  the  electrotonic 
currents. 

The  action  of  magnesium  indicates  that  the  effects 
produced  by  the  electrotonic  potentials  are  exerted 
through  the  medium  of  the  nerve  terminals  and  are 


100 

50 

c 
c 
o 

'-  5 


.»-  Anodic  Cathod'C  -». 


FIG.  17.  Effects  upon  the  frequency  of  miniature  e.p.p.'s  in 
rat  diaphragm  muscle  fibers  of  electrotonus  appHed  to  the 
phrenic  nerve.  Abscissae  show  the  intensity  of  applied  electro- 
tonic current  in  relative  units;  ordinates,  the  frequency  of 
miniature  e.p.p.'s  scaled  logarithmically.  Arrows  point  to 
frequencies  of  the  latter  observed  when  no  electrotonic  currents 
were  applied.  'Cathodic'  current  is  depolarizing  for  the  nerve 
terminals,  anodic'  is  hyperpolarizing.  A:  The  eflfects  of  the 
change  in  potential  were  essentially  symmetrical  on  the  loga- 
rithmic scale,  increa.sed  frequency  of  miniature  e.p.p.'s  with 
cathodic  and  decreased  frequency  with  anodic  current.  This 
was  the  most  frequently  encountered  result.  B:  open  circles, 
terminal  depolarization  was  much  more  efTective  than  hyper- 
polarization  in  changing  the  frequency  in  this  experiment; 
Jilled  circles,  the  same  inuscle  was  exposed  to  12  mmole  mag- 
nesium (normal  concentration  is  i  mmole).  The  frequency  of 
miniature  e.p.p.'s  became  essentially  independent  of  the  mem- 
brane potential  of  the  nerve  fibers.  [From  Liley  (137).] 


genuinely  synaptic  in  nature.  Other  tests  also  lead  to 
this  conclusion,  a)  The  electrotonic  effects  on  the 
miniature  e.p.p.'s  are  absent  in  muscle  fibers  where 
the  nerve  supply  is  cut  close  to  the  muscle  and  thereby 
made  inaccessible  to  the  electrotonic  currents.  This 
rules  out  the  possibility  that  the  current  flow  in  the 
muscle  fibers  themselves  caused  the  changed  rate  of 
miniature  e.p.p.'s.  b')  The  effect  of  the  electrotonus 
was  absent  in  endplates  that  were  more  than  a  few 
millimeters  from  the  site  of  applying  the  stimulus  to 
the  nerve.  Since  the  decay  of  electrotonically  spread 
potentials  must  be  rapid  in  the  terminal  nerve  fibers, 
this  result  indicates  that  the  change  in  rate  is  initiated 
by  effects  in  the  presynaptic  terminals.  These  experi- 
ments show  that  when  the  depolarization  produced 
by  a  nerve  impulse  arrives  at  or  near  the  presynaptic 
terminals,  their  secretory  activity  can  be  initiated  or 
augmented.  A  mechanism  coupling  the  presynaptic 
impulse  and  transmission  is  thus  provided. 

Some  additional  conclusions  may  be  deduced  from 
data  on  miniature  e.p.p.'s.  These  activities  increase  in 
frequency  approximately  lo-fold  for  15  mv  depolari- 
zation (137).  Therefore  a  spike,  though  lasting  only  a 
brief  time,  could  mobilize  the  rapid  release  of  a 
considerable  number  of  transmitter  packets  since  100 
mv  depolarization  might  increase  the  rate  of  '  spon- 
taneous' releases  some  10'  to  10*  times.  The  number 
of  packets  involved  in  an  e.p.p.  during  neuromuscular 
transmission  is  probably  about  10-  to  10^  times  the 
'quantal'  units  that  cause  the  miniature  e.p.p.'s  (52). 

Increase  in  the  rate  of  release  or  .secretion  of  the 
transmitter  at  the  presynaptic  terminals  is  obviously 
an  electrically  activated  event.  However,  the  response 
at  the  effector  terminals  probably  differs  Ijasically 
from  the  processes  that  generate  the  spike  of  the  con- 
ductile  membrane.  The  data  of  figure  1 7  were  ob- 
tained with  prolonged  applications  of  electrotonic 
currents.  The  sustained  increase  of  miniature  e.p.p.'s 
during  sustained  depolarization  therefore  indicates 
that  the  processes  leading  to  release  of  transmitter 
packets  are  not  subject  to  inactivation  as  is  the  sodium 
conductance  of  the  spike  generator. 

Gradation  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Probably  the  miniature  p.s.p.'s  are  small  only  be- 
cause the  area  involved  in  their  electrogenic  activity 
is  small  in  comparison  with  the  total  area  over  which 
the  emf  is  electrotonically  distributed.  .Suppose  that 
we  could  measure  the  change  in  potential  occurring 
at  a  single  isolated  site  which  valves  sodium  ions.  In 
the  resting  state  the  emf  across  that  site  will  be  the 


1.68 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


resting  potential,  approximately  the  value  of  the  po- 
tassium potential,  EsCgi,  112,  113).  When  the  valve 
■  opens'  the  emf  must  suddenly  change  from  the  resting 
value  towards  that  of  the  sodium  potential,  £>,-,,,  a 
change  to  internal  positivity  (113).  The  hypothetical 
'valve',  however,  is  located  in  a  physical  structure, 
the  membrane,  with  finite  resistance  and  capacity 
and  with  both  its  surfaces  bathed  in  saline  media. 
The  step-like  emf  of  the  generator  '  valve'  must  there- 
fore distrilauie  itself  electrotonically  o\er  an  area  hav- 
ing definite  electrical  properties,  becoming  a  potential 
change  reduced  in  magnitude  and  distorted  in  form 
(fig.  2).  The  simultaneous  activity  of  a  number  of 
"valves'  would  lead  to  an  increased  potential,  thus 
permitting  gradation  of  the  response  from  the  mini- 
mal observable  to  the  full  value  of  the  electrochemical 
potential.  Since  several  species  of  ions  are  invoked, 
the  maximum  p.s.p.  strikes  a  i)alance  ijetween  the 
different  electrochemical  potentials  (cf.  52,  60). 

Aferlianisms  of  Graded  Responsiveness 

The  most  detailed  data  are  available  on  graded 
responses  of  electrically  excitable  membrane  and,  al- 
though the  theory  of  their  production  is  still  rudi- 
mentary, the  same  general  process  will  probably  be 
found  to  apply  also  to  the  graded  responses  of  synaptic 
and  sensory  membrane  (94-96).  Graded  local  re- 
sponse is  usually  considered  to  be  merely  a  stage  in 
the  events  leading  to  the  regenerative  explosive  activ- 
ity which  results  in  the  spike  (113).  This  view  has 
been  invalidated  by  the  finding  (4,  6,  92)  that  under 
various  conditions  all-or-none  responsiveness  can  be 
converted  to  a  fully  graded  one.  Only  graded  re- 
sponses occur  in  dually-responsive  insect  muscle 
fibers  (37,  38)  and  probably  in  other  electrically 
excitable  membranes  as  well  (97)-  The  activity  may 
vary  from  the  minimal  observable  to  a  maximal 
response  closely  approximating  the  spike  in  amplitude 
and  form  (fig.  18;  cf.  fig.  21).  The  degree  of  graded 
responsiveness  is  not  controlled  by  the  membrane 
potential  as  it  is  considered   to  be  in  current  theory 

Figure  18  also  illustrates  how  an  altered  local 
excitatory  state  need  not  be  caused  by,  nor  reflected 
in,  a  changed  membrane  potential.  Whether  un- 
treated or  poisoned  with  a  drug,  the  single  cell  showed 
subliminally  enhanced  excitability  which  w-as  evi- 
denced during  an  interval  at  least  0.2  sec.  after  each 
subthreshold  stimulus.  The  cumulative  growth  of  this 
"excited'  state  in  the  untreated  cell  led  to  an  explosive 
manifestation,  the  spike.  After  the  cell  was  poisoned 


the  overt  manifestation  look  the  form  of  a  progres- 
sively larger  graded  response,  and  this  response 
approached  the  spike  in  amplitude. 

A  first  approximation  for  revising  theoretical  con- 
cepts (94,  95)  con'iiders  that  the  excitalile  memiirane 
is  composed  of  unit  areas.  Each  has  a  population  of 
electrogenic  units  (transducers,  valves,  etc.)  which 
differ  amongst  themselves  in  the  threshold  for  their 
excitation.  In  the  explosively  responsive  population 
the  thresholds  for  exciting  the  electrogenic  elements 
of  a  given  unit  area  are  probably  closely  similar. 
Dispersion  of  that  population  distribution  could  re- 
sult in  conver-^ion  of  all-or-none  responsi\eness  to  the 
graded  type. 

Transfer  of  Activity  From  Postsynaptic  Potentials  to 
Electrically  Excitable  .Membrane 

In  the  case  of  the  skeletal  muscle  endplate  or  the 
squid  giant  fiber  synapse  a  relatively  well-defined 
'  patch'  of  electrically  inexcitable  synaptic  membrane 
is  surrounded  by  electrically  excitable  structure.  In 
both  cells,  the  p.s.p.  is  simple,  only  of  the  depolarizing 
variety,  and  initiated  by  impulses  in  a  single  pre- 
synaptic fiber  (fig.  19).  The  p.s.p.  then  tends  to  be  of 
a  fixed  amplitude  and  in  these  two  systems  usually 
causes  sufficient  depolarization  of  the  contiguous 
electrically  excitable  membrane  to  generate  a  spike 
in  the  latter.  Essentially,  transmission  then  is  one-to- 
one,  each  impulse  of  the  prefiber  generating  a  post- 
junctional spike. 

Under  various  conditions,  for  example  upon  poison- 
ing an  endplate  with  rf-tubocurarine,  the  p.s.p.  de- 
creases in  amplitude  and,  when  the  depolarization 
falls  below  the  critical  firing  level,  no  spike  is  gener- 
ated (fig.  15).  The  transmission  block  may  be  over- 
come by  a  rapidly  repetitive  volley  of  neural  stimuli 
which  successively  generate  new  p.s.p.'s  or  a  local 
excitatory  state  before  the  previous  ha\e  disappeared. 
The  consequent  augmentation  of  depolarizing  elec- 
trogenesis  may  attain  the  critical  level  and  transmis- 
sion again  occurs.  This  general  phenomenon  of 
increased  effectiveness  of  repetitive  stimuli  is  known 
as  facilitation.  The  normally  occurring  p.s.p.  produced 
by  a  given  afferent  neural  stimulus  may  not  be  suf- 
ficiently large  to  evoke  a  spike.  Repetitive  stimuli  in 
this  case  can  summate  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  and 
facilitation  is  then  also  manifested,  the  summed  de- 
polarization initiating  a  spike. 

In  the  context  of  the  electrophysiological  mecha- 
nism, a  facilitated  overt  respon.se  (e.g.  of  a  muscle) 
may   be   produced   by   two   fundamentally  different 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


169 


A. 


JV. 


A. 


B 


-V 


/v. 


-V 


.^ 


JV 


.^. 


.^. 


.^^ 


-^ 


.^ 


.^ 


-^ 


8' 


.^. 


FIG.  18.  All-oi-none  and  graded  responsiveness  in  an  eel 
clectroplaque.  Two  traces  are  recorded  simultaneously,  re- 
peated at  the  rate  of  5  per  sec.  The  upper  longer  trace  of  each 
set  is  the  zero  base  line  for  an  internal  microelectrode.  It  also 
carries  the  monitoring  signal  of  a  stimulus  applied  to  the  cell 
and  shows  that  the  stimulus  strength  remained  constant  in 
each  of  the  two  series.  The  lower  trace  of  each  set  is  that  of  the 
potential  recorded  with  the  microelectrode.  The  distances  be- 
tween the  two  represent  the  resting  potential,  about  70  mv. 
The  weak  stimulus  in  A,  before  the  cell  was  treated  with  drug, 
at  first  produced  only  a  subthreshold  electrotonic  depolariza- 
tion. The  seventh  repetition  of  the  stimulus  is  followed  by  a 
spike.  The  shorter  latency  at  which  successive  spikes  then  de- 
velop indicates  continued  growth  of  excitability  and  its  per- 
sistence through  the  200  msec,  intervals  between  stimuli.  The 
resting  potential  remained  unchanged.  B  and  B' .  The  sequence 
of  growth  in  response  in  the  cell  after  84  min.  exposure  to  500 
Mg  per  ml  of  physostigmine.  The  resting  potential  was  not 
affected  by  the  drug,  which  eliminated  synaptic  excitability 
and  converted  the  all-or-none  response  of  the  electrically 
excitable  membrane  component  to  graded  responsiveness.  The 
testing  stimulus  was  slightly  stronger  than  before  applying  the 
drug,  and  the  first  trace  seen  (upper  set  of  S)  evoked  a  distinct. 


synaptic  processes.  The  one  descrif)ed  just  above  is 
summation  where  each  successive  p.s.p.  is  no  larger 
(cf.  fig.  27.4),  and  may  indeed  be  smaller,  than  its 
predecessor.  The  excitatory  action  leading  to  the 
overt  effect  would  be  the  increased  total  depolariza- 
tion produced  b\-  the  summed  effects  of  the  repeated 
p.s.p.'s.  The  overt  effect  would  appear  as  a  facilita- 
tion because  of  the  profound  functional  difference 
between  the  local  processes  at  the  motoneuronal  or 
neuromuscular  synaptic  junction  and  their  production 
of  an  explosive  propagated  spike  which  triggers  the 
contractile  mechanism. 

Essentially  the  same  overt  result,  but  an  activity 
involving  more  complex  synaptic  processes,  would 
(jccur  if  the  successive  p.s.p.'s  augmented  as  a  result 
of  the  repetitive  stimulation.  This  synaptic  facilitation 
will  be  discussed  further  in  relation  to  heterosynaptic 
and  homosynaptic  excitatory  phenomena  (p.  184).  It 
would  seem  to  involve  augmented  responsiveness  of 
the  synaptic  membrane  to  the  transmitter  agent,  the 
converse  to  the  decreased  responsiveness  in  desensiti- 
zation.  As  noted  in  that  connection,  defacilitation 
probably  is  ascribable  to  desensitization.  Both  facili- 
tation and  defacilitation,  however,  may  be  only 
apparent  effects  on  the  synaptic  membrane,  their  real 
cause  residing  elsewhere.  For  example,  facilitation 
could  result  from  successively  larger  quantities  of 
transmitter  released  from  the  presynaptic  terminals. 
The  converse,  progressive  exhaustion  of  the  trans- 
iTiitter  and  decrease  of  the  amount  emitted  at  each 
impulse,  would  lead  to  defacilitation. 

As  is  also  the  case  with  other  electrical  stimuli,  the 
depolarizing  p.s.p.  first  evokes  a  graded  local  response 
of  the  electrically  excitable  membrane  (4)  and  the 
two  depolarizations  then  sum  to  cause  the  explosive 
response  of  the  spike  (figs.  3,  7).  The  addition  of 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  to  the  depolarizing  diminishes 
the  magnitude  of  the  latter  and  its  excitatory  effect. 
If  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.  then  falls  below  the  critical 
level,  a  spike  is  no  longer  elicited  and  the  effect  of 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  is  therefore  inhibitory.  It 
should  be  noted  that  inhibition  may  occur  even 
though  considerable  depolarization  is  still  generated. 
In  other  words,  the  countervailing  inhibitory  p.s.p. 


though  small,  graded  response.  During  the  course  of  repetitive 
stimulation  at  5  per  sec.  the  response  grew,  at  first  gradually 
and  then  more  rapidly,  indicating  that  the  rise  of  excitability 
is  non-linear.  The  series  illustrated  ends  before  the  response 
could  grow  to  an  amplitude  as  large  as  that  of  the  spike,  but 
in  other  experiments  this  was  observed.  [From  .Mtamirano  el 
al.  (6).l 


1 70 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLCKJY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGV 


I     I     I     I     I     I     I 

msec 

FIG.  19.  Synaptic  transfer  in  squid  giant  axons.  The  incom- 
ing presynaptic  spike  elicits  only  a  small  membrane  potential 
change  in  the  postsynaptic  cell.  The  p.s.p.  arises  after  a  brief 
latency  and,  if  it  attains  the  critical  firing  level,  elicits  a  spike. 
[From  Bullock  &  Hagiwara  (32).] 


need  not  be  as  large  as  is  the  excitatory  one.  It  must 
only  be  large  enough  to  decrease  the  depolarizing 
p.s.p.  below  the  critical  firing  level  for  the  spike,  but 
it  can  then  produce  dramatic  effects  since  the  absence 
of  conductile  activity  eliminates  further  transfer  to 
other  cells  and  results  in  the  disappearance  of  distant 
actions  within  the  organism. 


Synaptic  Delay 

Synaptic  latency,  which  was  discussed  above,  in- 
volves only  the  activity  of  the  presynaptic  terminals 
and  the  response  of  electrically  inexcitable  synaptic 
membrane.  Synaptic  delay  includes  not  only  the 
latency  but  also  the  utilization  time  of  electrical 
excitability.  This  last  involves  the  duration  of  the  rise 
of  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.  and  of  whatever  further 
depolarization  this  may  develop  in  its  excitatory  ac- 
tion on  electrically  excitable  sites,  and  the  consequent 
time  that  is  required  for  the  p.s.p.  (and  the  local 
response)  to  reach  the  critical  level  for  evoking  a 
spike.  The  rise  time  of  the  p.s.p.  for  this  level  may  be 
brief,  about  o.  i  to  0.3  msec.  (figs.  6,  12),  but  can  be 
much  longer  (figs.  7,  9),  particularly  if  the  depolariz- 
ing p.s.p.  is  liminal  for  discharge  of  the  spike.  Tem- 
poral summation  or  facilitation,  in  which  repetitively 
evoked  depolarization  becomes  larger,  may  then  de- 
crease the  utilization  time  and  thereby  shorten  the 
synaptic  delay  (cf.  140).  The  shortening  might  also 
occur  because  of  decreased  synaptic  latency  or 
heightened  synaptic  excitability,  effects  which  are 
discussed  in  the  next  section  of  this  chapter. 

The  existence  of  synaptic  delay  has  been  a.scribed 
chiefly  to  slowed  conduction  of  the  afferent  impulse 
in   the   fine   terminals   of  the   presynaptic   fibers  (cf. 


57,  140).  That  explanation  is  no  longer  tenable. 
Strong  electrical  stimuli  directly  applied  to  the  inner- 
vated surface  of  the  eel  electroplaque,  and  therefore 
to  the  nerve  terminals,  nevertheless  cause  a  neurally 
evoked  response  always  after  a  considerable  synaptic 
latency  (fig.  6).  Further  evidence  may  be  derived 
from  figure  ig  and  other  data  of  similar  nature  which 
show  that  the  presynaptic  spike  arrives  at  the  synap- 
tic surface  somewhat  before  the  p.s.p.  is  elicited.  Thus, 
.synaptic  latency  and  the  utilization  time  involved 
in  the  rise  of  the  p.s.p.  to  the  critical  firing  level  are 
probably   the   major  factors   in   synaptic   delay. 

Sujifrjuisition  of  Pustsyriafitic  Potentials  and  Spikes 

The  electrically  inexcitable  generators  of  p.s.p. 's  act 
independently  of  and  in  parallel  with  the  electrically 
excitable  membrane  that  produces  the  spike  (4, 
48,  71).  Thus,  a  p.s.p.  can  be  evoked  during  the 
spike,  when  a  second  response  of  the  electrically 
excitable  membrane  is  impossible  due  to  its  absolute 
refractoriness  (figs.  6,  7).  However,  the  combined 
response  depends  upon  the  prevailing  electrochem- 
ical conditions  of  the  cell.  The  p.s.p.  may  subtract 
from  as  well  as  add  to  the  spike,  the  former  occiu'ring 
when  the  spike  itself  carries  the  membrane  potential 
into  the  region  at  which  the  p.s.p.  reverses  as  de- 
scribed above  (48,  136;  cf  97).  The  conclusion  that 
the  spike  under  certain  conditions  wipes  out  the 
p.s.p.  (cf  60,  p.  30  ff)  may  therefore  require  revision. 
A  complicating  factor  that  ma)'  explain  these  find- 
ings of  Eccles  and  his  colleagues  is  the  distortion 
produced  in  the  spike  when  the  latter  is  elicited  in  a 
depolarized  electrically  excitable  membrane  (cf 
95).  An  "undershoot'  of  apparently  hyperpolarizing 
phase  then  terminates  the  spike,  even  though  it  is 
absent  in  the  response  evoked  at  the  normal  resting 
potential  of  the  membrane  (fig.  10;  cf  60,  fig.  16). 
The  distortion  is  probably  due  (95)  to  excess  of  po- 
tassium conductance  over  the  sodium  conductance 
as  in  .squid  giant  axons  (113).  This  excess  would  be 
caused  h\  increased  sodium  inactivation  produced 
by  the  depolarization. 

The  foregoing  remarks  indicate  that  electrical  and 
physiological  conditions  of  the  soma  membrane  affect 
the  recording  of  celhdar  potentials.  The  soma,  how- 
ever, is  only  one  part  of  the  cell,  although  it  is  the 
one  most  easily  accessible  to  microelectrodcs.  Even 
in  neurons  without  dendrites,  as  is  the  case  in  tissue- 
cultured  dorsal  root  ganglion  cells,  the  intracellularly 
recorded  response  to  stimuli  may  take  on  complex 
forms  (42).  This  indicates  that  activits    in  and   the 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


171 


properties  of  the  axon  contribute  to  the  potential  re- 
corded   from    the    soma. 

The  nature  and  degree  of  excitability  may  be  dif- 
ferent in  various  parts  of  the  soma  and  dendrites. 
Thus  the  soma  may  be  electrically  inexcitable  (ry, 
33,  80,  186,  189,  190).  The  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
or  generator  potentials  cv'oked  at  the  soma  excite 
spikes  at  electrically  excitable  regions  some  distance 
from  the  cell  body.  The  superficial  portions  of  apical 
dendrites  in  the  cat  cortex  are  not  electrically  excit- 
able (loy,  165),  As  mentioned  earlier,  the  receptor 
portions  of  various  sensory  cells  are  electrically  in- 
excitable  and  for  this  reason  are  capable  of  develop- 
ing   a    sustained    generator    potential.^ 

Recent  evidence  (7,  17,  43,  82)  also  indicates  that 
different  portions  of  electrically  excitable  mem- 
branes of  the  cell  body  may  have  different  thresh- 
olds. The  ■  initial  segment'  of  the  motoneuron  (cf. 
60)  in  the  cat  (82)  and  toad  (7)  responds  first  to  an 
electrical  stimulus  and  gives  rise  to  the  early  part  of 
the  antidromic  spike  (fig.  I2^'-F')-  The  spike  of  the 
rest  of  the  cell  body  (if  the  latter  is  electrically  ex- 
citable) occurs  slightly  later,  the  delay  giving  rise 
to  a  slight  break  in  the  recorded  response. 

In  addition  to  these  apparent  inhomogeneities  in 
the  excitability  of  different  parts  of  the  soma  and 
dendrites,  slowed  conductile  spread,  separate  loci  of 
origin  for  spike  and  p.s.p.'s  and  different  loci  for 
depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  are  all 
factors  that  may  contribute  to  variations  in  the 
recorded  response  of  the  cell.  Many  variations  can 
be  theoretically  deduced,  but  their  analysis  is  beyond 
the  present  scope. 

'Retinal  receptors  in  lisli  (184)  provide  an  interesting  new 
example  (102).  Their  electrical  response  is  probably  generated 
in  cells  other  than  the  primary  visual  cells  (cones).  The  re- 
sponse is  a  sustained  clectrogenesis.  In  some  cells  it  is  only 
hyperpolarizing,  in  others  depolarization  is  also  developed, 
depending  upon  the  wavelength  of  the  stimulating  light.  The 
amplitudes  of  the  responses  are  graded,  not  only  with  the  in- 
tensity of  the  light  stimulus  but  also  with  its  spectral  composi- 
tion. These  characteristics  of  electrically  inexcitable  activity  are 
produced  apparently  in  the  absence  of  spikes,  but  the  electro- 
genesis,  both  hyperpolarizing  and  depolarizing,  affects  spike 
production  in  other  conductile  elements.  It  has  been  suggested 
(102)  that  these  electrogenic  cells  (probably  horizontal  or  bi- 
polar cells  or  both)  are  excited  by  transmitter  agents  released 
by  photochemically  activated  cones.  The  clectrogenesis,  in 
which  an  electrically  excitable  component  is  lacking,  is  in  turn 
associated  with  secretory  activity  as  in  electrically  inexcitable 
gland  cells.  The  secretory  products  acting  upon  the  retinal 
ganglion  cells  evoke  neuronal  activity  of  the  latter,  probably 
including  excitatory  and  inhibitory  p.s.p.'s  which  lead  to 
patterns  of  spike  activity  seen  in  the  optic  nerve  fibers. 


GENERAL  AND   COMPARATIVE   PHYSIOLOGY   OF  SYNAPSES 

Forms  and  Magnitudes  of  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Viewed  as  the  nonregenerative  responses  of  elec- 
trically inexcitable  membrane,  the  forms  and  mag- 
nitudes of  the  p.s.p.'s  may  be  expected  to  have 
rather  simple  relations  to  their  excitants.  The  availa- 
ble experimental  data  are  as  yet  rather  scanty,  but 
they  do  permit  some  general  conclusions  (cf.  60,  97). 

As  a  first  approximation,  the  degree  of  synaptic 
transducer  action  reflected  in  the  rate  and  amount 
of  clectrogenesis  may  be  considered  to  be  roughly 
proportional  to  the  quantity  of  excitant.  A  brief  jet 
of  labile  transmitter  or  activating  drug  causes  a 
Ijrief  response  while  the  continued  availability  of 
the  excitant  causes  a  sustained  clectrogenesis.  The 
duration  of  the  p.s.p.  in  the  first  case  will  be  deter- 
mined by  the  time  course  of  the  transducer  action 
initiated  by  the  excitant  (cf.  also  53,  127,  and  papers 
cited  there).  However,  the  responses  will  be  dis- 
torted by  the  electrical  circuit  properties  of  the 
membranes.  Thus,  the  rising  and  falling  phases  of 
the  p.s.p.  may  reflect  this  distortion  which  produces 
a  slowing  such  as  occurs  in  electrotonic  propagation 
(fig.  2).  The  rise  of  the  p.s.p.  should  be  slowed  less 
than  its  fall  since  the  former  occurs  when  the  mem- 
brane resistance  and  time  constant  are  relatively 
low.  This  is  the  case  experimentally  as  numerous 
figures  in  this  chapter  indicate.  The  falling  phase 
probably  bears  some  relation  to  the  time  constant 
of  the  membrane  (cf.  60),  lasting  longer  when  the 
time  constant  is  larger,  like  the  ballistic  response  of  a 
slow  galvanometer  to  a  brief  current.  The  relation, 
however,  does  not  appear  to  be  a  simple  one  (95, 
97),  and  the  duration  of  the  p.s.p.  probably  reflects 
importanth  intrinsic  time  courses  of  transducer  ac- 
tions. 

The  duration  of  the  p.s.p.  caused  by  a  single 
neural  volley  differs  considerably  in  the  various  types 
of  cells.  The  p.s.p.'s  of  squid  giant  axons  and  of  eel 
electroplaques  last  only  about  2  msec.  (figs.  3,  19), 
those  of  Aplysia  giant  neurons  (fig.  7)  or  cat  salivary 
glands  (fig.  20)  may  persist  for  nearly  i  sec.  The 
e.p.p.'s  and  p.s.p.'s  of  other  neurons  have  inter- 
mediate durations.  In  .some  cases,  physostigmine 
and  prostigmine  both  prolong  the  p.s.p.,  this  effect 
probably  involving  the  prolongation  of  the  life  of  the 
transmitter,  acetylcholine,  by  inactivation  of  cho- 
linesterase  (cf.  52,  53,  60,  68).  Some  of  the  quater- 
nary ammonium  compounds  also  prolong  p.s.p.'s 
(cf  52)  and  these  actions  may  be  caused  by  direct 


172 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Fio.  20.  Different  types  of  electrical 
activity  in  cat  salivary  gland  cells. 
Depolarization  shown  as  downward 
deflection  in  these  records.  A:  Type  I 
cells  produce  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
which  are  graded  with  strength  of  the 
stimulus.  Single  shocks  to  chorda 
tympani  evoke  p.s.p.'s  which  last  about 

I  sec.  B:  Type  I  cells  produce  only 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  to  excitation  ot 
the  sympathetic  {upper  iigna!)  or  para- 
sympathetic {lower  signal)  nerves.  How- 
ever, the  latencies  and  magnitudes  of 
the   p.s.p.'s   differ   somewhat.    C:  Type 

II  cells  develop  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
on  stimulating  the  chorda  tympani  and 
depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  through  their 
sympathetic  innervation.  D:  Type  III 
cells  (which  may  be  myoepithelial 
elements  of  the  ducts)  respond  only  with 
depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  to  parasympa- 
thetic {above)  or  sympathetic  {below) 
stimulation.  The  resting  potential, 
about  —80  mv,  is  large  in  comparison 
with  that  of  Type  I  or  II  cells  and 
resembles  that  of  muscle  fibers.  E: 
Type  I  cells  respond  with  hyperpolari- 
zation  to  epinephrine,  acetylcholine 
and  pilocarpine.  [From  Lundberg 
(144).]  F:  The  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p. 
of  the  gland  cell  is  remarkably  insensi- 
ti\e  to  changes  of  the  membrane  po- 
tential. The  resting  potential  was  30 
mv.  [From  Lundberg  (145).] 


B 


mV 

10+- 

Oh_ 

2sec 

-60 

- 

-50 

- 

-40 

- 

-30 

- 

-20 

- 

2  sec 


-40 


-120 
-80 
-40 
-    0 


2  sec 


-50  - 
-40  - 
-30  - 
-20  - 


2sec. 


l/^g  adr. 


O.l/ig    och. 


-120 
-100- 

-  80 
-60 
-40- 

-  20 

-  0 


0.5 /ig   pilocar. 


effects  upon  the  kinetics  of  the  ionic  'valving'  of  tiie 
transducer  action. 

The  maximum  attainable  amplitudes  of  p.s.p.'s 
are  probably  determined  by  electrochemical  condi- 
tions as  described  in  a  previous  section  of  this 
chapter,  but  these  need  not  be  identical  for  different 
varieties  of  cells.  Thus,  most  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
reach  a  limit  set  by  the  most  negative  electrochemi- 
cal ionic  species,  but  hyperpolarizing  electrogenesis 
of  glands  can  occur  in  the  face  of  very  high  internal 
negativity  (fig.  20).  These  differences  reinforce  the 
conclusion  (91,    105)  that  electrical  activity  of  bio- 


logical membranes  may  involve  a  variety  of  mcch 
anisms,  some  of  which  are  not  yet  understood. 

Postjunctianal    Cells    with    Dfpolaii'ing 
Postsynaptic    Potentials 

As  noted  above,  some  cells  though  not  electrically 
excitaijle  respond  with  depolarization  to  neural  or 
chemical  stimuli.  Of  general  interest  are  electrically 
inexcitable  invertebrate  and  vertebrate  muscle 
hbers,  such  as  the  '  slow'  muscle  fibers  of  the  frog 
(fig.  4i4).  They  are  diffusely  innervated  and  neural 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


173 


Stimuli  give  rise  to  graded  summative  depolariza- 
tions. These  diffusely  generated  depolarizations  can 
act  as  stimuli  for  the  contractile  mechanism,  causing 
localized  graded  contractions  (34,  35,  132). 

Some  salivary  gland  cells  also  generate  only  de- 
polarizing p.s.p.'s  (fig.  20)  and  these  are  produced 
by  stimulation  of  either  the  sympathetic  or  para- 
sympathetic nerves  (144).  Chemical  stimulation  by 
epinephrine,  pilocarpine  or  acetylcholine  then  all 
cause  the  same  type  of  electrogenesis,  but  it  is  not 
known  whether  all  the  excitants  activate  a  single 
variety  of  electrogenic  membrane  or  whether  there 
are  distinct,  although  similarly  electrogenic,  cholino- 
ceptive  and  adrenoceptive  components.  As  is  the 
case  with  the  electrically  inexcitable  muscle  fibers, 
the  synaptic  electrogenesis  of  gland  cells  is  also  asso- 
ciated with  and  itself  probably  effects  other  cellular 
activity,  in  this  case  secretion.'^ 

Torpedo  and  Raia  elcctroplaqucs  also  generate 
only  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  but  not  to  electrical  stimuli 
(95).  The  cells  which  are  derived  from  skeletal 
muscles  therefore  are  in  reality  constituted  from  end- 
plates.  A  specialization  of  the.se  and  other  electro- 
plaques  permits  series  additions  of  the  voltages  pro- 
duced by  each  cell;  hence  the  electric  organs  generate 
considerable  voltage.  The  discharges  are  under  con- 
trol of  the  nervous  system  and  in  .some  forms  this 
may  be  useful  for  protection  or  aggression.  The 
p.s.p.'s  are  Ijrief  in  Torj>edo  but  long-lasting  in  Raia. 

Vertebrate  skeletal  muscle  fibers  of  the  '  twitch' 
system  and  autonomic  ganglia  combine  depolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  and  spike-generating  membrane  (cf  figs.  9, 
27),  but  the  autonomic  neuron  may  also  produce 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  since  there  are  indications 
thai   inhil)ilion   may  occur  (64,    134).    In   both  cases 

'  It  was  noted  earlier  (p.  154)  that  bioelectric  responses  of 
transmissional  and  conductile  processes  are  essentially  passive 
events  resulting  from  the  mo%ement  of  ions  in  obedience  to 
charged  electrochemical  equilibrium  states.  The  change  from 
one  state  to  another  is  the  active  phenomenon,  due  to  specific 
processes,  transducer  actions  which  are  the  responses  of  excit- 
able membrane  to  appropriate  stimuli.  In  gland  cells,  the  se- 
cretory activity  of  the  output  component  (fig.  16)  probably 
occurs  at  membrane  .structures  that  are  intimately  mingled 
with  those  of  the  input  component.  .Secretory  electrogenesis 
thus  is  probably  superimposed  on  the  p.s.p.'s  of  the  transducer 
input,  and  this  is  suggested  also  by  the  independence  of  the 
gland  electrogenesis  from  electrochemical  conditions  (fig.  20 
E  and  F).  In  some  respects,  therefore,  electrogenesis  of  gland 
cells  may  differ  from  that  of  'pure'  p.s.p.'s  of  neurons  or  end- 
plates.  The  details  of  these  diflferences  cannot  now  be  specified 
since  little  is  known  about  the  nature  of  active  transport 
mechanisms,  such  as  arc  probably  involved  in  secretion. 


the  p.s.p.'s  have  much  longer  durations  than  do  the 
spikes.  In  muscle  fibers  the  spike  energizes  the  proc- 
esses of  contraction  by  a  mechanism  that  is  not  yet 
known  (cf  121).  Eel  electroplaques  also  generate 
both  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  and  spikes  (cf  figs.  3,  6, 
13),  but  the  contractile  machinery  is  missing  in  these 
modified  muscle  fibers.  Eel  electroplaques,  like 
neurons,  are  diffusely  innervated  Isy  many  nerve 
fibers.  Since  the  area  of  their  innervated  surface  is 
more  than  10  mm-,  their  study  has  provided  some 
data  that  are  not  readily  obtained  with  the  much 
smaller  nerve  cells.  The  results,  however,  very  prob- 
ably apply  to  the  general  case  of  synaptic  transmis- 
sion as  will  be  described  below  (cf.  95). 

Postjunctional  Cells  with  Hyperpolarizing 
Postsynaptic  Potentials 

If  cells  capable  of  generating  spikes  were  endowed 
only  with  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s,  transmfssional 
excitation  of  the  electrically  excitable  responses  would 
not  occur,  for  in  all  cases  known  the  spike  is  triggered 
by  depolarization.  Thus,  it  may  be  expected  that 
cells  in  which  solely  hyperpolarizing  synaptic  elec- 
trogenesis occurs  would  be  of  restricted  functional 
significance.  From  intracellular  recordings  two  cases 
are  known  and  in  neither  are  spikes  generated.  These 
are  salivary  gland  cells  (144,  146;  cf  fig.  20)  and  L- 
cells  of  the  fish  retina  (102,  184,  191 ;  cf  al.so  footnote 
4,  above).  As  noted  earlier,  the  memijrane  trans- 
ducer actions  and  electrochemical  effects  of  hyper- 
polarization  are  consistent  with  secretory  activity; 
hence  neurally  evoked  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  of 
glands  have  functional  validity. 

Postjunctional  Cells  ivith  Both   Types  of 
Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Two  varieties  may  be  expected  and  both  types 
occur:  /)  electrically  ine.xcitable  cells  which  do  not 
generate  spikes  and  2)  cells  which  produce  spikes  as 
well  as  the  p.s.p.'s.  A  clear  case  of  the  former  is 
found  in  some  salivary  gland  cells  (fig.  20)  in  which 
each  type  of  synpatic  electrogenesis  is  probably  asso- 
ciated with  a  different  form  of  secretory  activity.  The 
different  p.s.p.'s  are  specifically  produced  by  stimu- 
lation of  the  two  autonomic  nerve  supplies.  Stim- 
ulation by  cholinomimetic  and  adrenomimetic 
substances  evokes  oppositely  signed  electrogenesis. 
The  R-G  and  Y-B  cells  of  fish  retina  also  produce 


174 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing  potentials  without 
spikes  (184). 

Some  invertebrate  muscle  filjers  possess  dual 
synaptic  activity  (72,  73)  and  it  has  been  suggested 
(96)  that  vertebrate  smooth  muscle  may  also  belong 
to  this  category.  If  the  fibers  are  not  electrically 
excitable,  the  contractions  caused  by  their  depolariz- 
ing p.s.p.'s  would  be  local,  as  in  frog  "slow'  muscle 
fibers  (34,  35,  132).  The  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
would  serve  the  function  of  diminishing  or  regulating 
the  degree  of  the  mechanical  response  by  decreasing 
the  depolarizations  of  the  'excitatory'  p.s.p.'s. 

By  far  the  most  prominent  class  are  the  cells  in 
which  spikes  as  well  as  the  two  kinds  of  p.s.p.'s  are 
generated.  Most,  and  perhaps  all,  neurons  of  the 
vertebrate  central  nervous  system  probably  belong 
to  this  group  (cf.  59,  60,  158,  159,  161 -167).  The 
hyperpolarizing  and  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  appear  to 
have  nearly  identical  durations  and  the  superposi- 
tion of  the  two  p.s.p.'s  may  decrease  membrane 
depolarization  sufficiently  to  eliminate  spike  produc- 
tion  by   an   orthodromic   excitatory   pathway.    This 


interaction  of  depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  adds  to  the  variety  and  flexibility  of  integra- 
tive activity  within  the  central  nervous  system.  The 
effects  are  achieved  not  only  by  relatively  simple 
algebraic  summation  of  the  potentials  but  also  by 
the  interplay  of  more  subtle  factors  which  will  be 
described  in  the  next  section  of  this  chapter. 

Fast  and  Slaw  Respunses  of  Iinrrtebratt'  Muscles 

The  muscle  fillers  of  some  insects  and  Crustacea 
(cf.  116,  117)  are  known  to  be  electrically  excitable, 
but  they  also  respond  diff^erently  to  stimulation  of 
different  excitatory  nerves  (fig.  21).  Their  'fast'  in- 
nervation, which  may  be  constituted  of  one  or  several 
nerve  fibers,  produces  large  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s 
upon  which  is  superimposed  a  spike-like  response, 
often  showing  a  small  overshoot.  Stimulation  of  the 
'slow'  nerve  fiber  leads  to  a  small  depolarizing  p.s.p. 
Upon  this  there  may  develop  various  gradations  of 
the  electrically  excitable  response.  The  mechanical 
acti\ities    are    also    different.    The    fast    nerve    fiber 


mV 


FIG.  21.  Different  responses  produced  in  insect  muscle  fibers  on  stimulating  their  fast  and  slow 
innervation.  Intracellular  recording  from  e.xtensor  tibiae  of  the  mesothoracic  leg  of  Schistocerca  gri'- 
garia.  A:  The  responses  of  six  different  muscle  fibers,  first  to  stimulation  of  the  fast  nerve  fiber  and 
then  the  slow.  In  all  but  one  muscle  fiber,  the  fast  response  developed  an  overshoot.  .^K  notch  on  the 
response  of  fiber  ii  indicates  the  level  on  the  p.s.p.  out  of  which  the  spike-like  activity  developed. 
In  fiber  ;,  as  in  about  50  per  cent  of  the  muscle  fibers,  no  response  resulted  on  stimulating  the  slow 
nerve  fiber.  Various  grades  of  activity  are  shown  in  the  other  examples.  In  three  of  these  (iii,  iv,  r) 
the  p.s.p.  was  large  enough  to  evoke  some  local  response  of  the  electrically  excitable  membrane.  B: 
Three  examples  of  facilitation  of  the  p.s.p.'s  by  repetitive  stimulation  of  the  slow  nerve  at  about 
30  per  sec.  The  augmented  p.s.p.'s  evoked  larger  pulsatile  local  responses,  and  in  one  case  (i.v)  an 
overshoot  was  obtained.  Time  and  amplitude  calibration  in  (0  apply  to  all  records.  [From  Hoyle 
(1 16).] 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


'75 


evokes  a  brisk  twitch  or  a  maximal  tetanus.  The 
slow  fiber  calls  forth  small  contractions  which  may 
grow  slowly  during  repetitive  stimulation. 

The  apparent  paradox  that  depolarization,  an 
electrical  and  unspecific  stimulus,  can  evoke  differ- 
ent forms  of  response  in  electrically  excitable  mem- 
brane has  been  resolved  by  the  finding  (37,  38)  that 
the  membrane  of  the  muscle  fibers  of  the  grass- 
hopper, Romalea  microptera,  though  electrically  ex- 
citable, responds  only  with  graded  activity.  Other 
physiological  and  anatomical  circumstances  co- 
operate with  this  normally  occurring  graded  respon- 
siveness. The  different  nerve  fibers  evoke  two  degrees 
of  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  in  the  electrically  inexcitable 
synaptic  membrane.  The  p.s.p.'s  evoked  by  the  fast 
nerve  fiber  may  be  larger  because  of  greater  synaptic 
potency  of  the  'fast'  transmitter  system  than  in  that 
of  the  slow  fiber  (e.g.  a  different  agent,  a  higher 
concentration  of  transmitter,  closer  approximation 
of  the  pre-  and  postsynaptic  membrane  or  larger 
area  of  synaptic  contact).  However,  another  alterna- 
tive is  that  the  membrane  sites  engaged  by  the 
terminals  of  the  different  fibers  are  different.  The 
combination  of  graded  p.s.p.'s  and  electrically 
excitable  local  responses  is  abetted  by  the  closeness 
of  synaptic  terminations.  The  terminals  of  the  fast 
nerve  fiber,  spaced  as  close  as  40/j  apart,  can  each 
evoke  large  local  responses  of  the  electrically  ex- 
citable membrane.  This  graded  activity,  summing 
its  depolarizing  actions,  can  then  evoke  maximal  re- 
sponses which  have  the  appearance  of  spikes.  The 
associated  contraction  is  a  twitch.  The  smaller 
p.s.p.'s  of  the  slow  response  can  be  graded  in  various 
proportions  and  can  evoke  local  response  of  various 
degrees.  The  resulting  contractions  are  also  graded. 

The  mechanisms  involved  in  the  dual  responses 
of  muscle  fibers  are  instructive  for  several  reasons. 
Dual  responsiveness  is  probably  present  in  muscles 
of  animals  quite  low  in  the  evolutionarv  scale  (i  17), 
and  this  suggests  that  electrically  excitable  mem- 
brane, like  the  sen.sory  or  synaptic,  was  originally 
gradedly  responsivp.  The  ability  to  develop  spikes 
then  would  have  been  a  later  evolutionary  stage  (2 1 , 
103).  Dual  responsiveness  also  represents  an  ex- 
ceedingly useful  mode  of  activity  for  arthropods  for 
their  muscles  are  limited  in  number.  The  size  of  the 
muscles  and  therefore  also  the  numijer  of  their  fibers 
are  limited  by  the  exigencies  of  the  exoskeleton.  The 
number  of  nerve  fibers  is  also  rather  small.  Despite 
these  limitations  arthropods  can  manipulate  their 
joints  intricately  and  with  precision  and  carry  out 
locomotion    with    great   dispatch    and    vigor.    These 


different  aspects  of  movement  are  all  achieved  with 
an  economy  of  means  because  of  special  responsive 
mechanisms  and  anatomical  conditions. 


PHARMACOLOGICAL    PROPERTIES    OF    SYNAPSES 
.^ND    THEIR    PHYSIOLOGIC.-VL    CONSEQUENCES 

The  discussion  in  this  part  of  the  present  chapter 
will  be  limited  to  vertebrate  synapses,  concerning 
which  information  is  more  extensive  than  on  in- 
vertebrate structures.  However,  the  pharmacology 
of  the  electrically  inexcitable  sensory  membrane  of 
the  crayfish  stretch  receptor  probably  parallels  that 
of  synapses  in  the  cat  brain  (96).  This  suggests 
that  in  their  general  aspects  the  pharmacological 
properties  of  vertebrate  and  invertebrate  synapses 
will  be  similar  in  principle,  although,  perhaps, 
invoKing  different  chemical  suijstances.  In  crustacean 
neuromuscular  synapses  and  in  the  inhibitory 
synapses  of  the  stretch  receptors  the  actions  of  amino 
acid  drugs  parallel  to  a  degree  the  effects  of  these 
substances  in  cat  brain  (cf.  99,  163,  and  below). 
However,  other  invertebrate  synapses  appear  to 
have  no  pharmacological  relation  to  vertebrate 
synapses  (cf.  99 j. 

Classification  of  Drug  Actions 

Depending  upon  the  theoretical  approach  and  the 
experimental  emphasis,  several  varieties  of  classifica- 
tion have  arisen.  Thus,  drugs  have  been  grouped  as 
'mimetics'  or  'lytics',  graded  according  to  the  degree 
to  which  they  mimic  or  block  the  action  of  nerve 
impulses,  or  sometimes  of  a  standard  comparison 
substance  (cf.  8).  Particularly  in  describing  effects  of 
drugs  on  the  more  complex  synaptic  systems  (chiefly 
of  the  central  nervous  system  but  also  those  of  smooth 
muscle)  substances  have  been  classified  as  'excitants' 
(or  'stimulants')  and  'inhibitors'  (or  'depressants'). 
For  example,  since  both  pentylenetetrazol  (Metra- 
zol)  and  strychnine  are  convulsant  agents,  both 
are  classified  as  stimulants  of  the  central  nervous 
system  (cf.  85).  Recently  (cf.  156)  the  drugs  acting 
upon  the  peripheral  cholinoceptive  synapses  of 
skeletal  muscle  and  autonomic  ganglia  have  been 
classified  as  'depolarizing'  or  as  'nondepolarizing, 
competitive,  antagonistic  inhibitors'  of  the  latter. 
This  classification  also  applies  to  the  simple  depo- 
larizing synapses  of  the  eel  electroplaques  (table  2). 

An  extension  of  this  classification  (table  3)  has 
proved  experimentally  and  analytically  more  useful 


176 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


TABLE  2.  Range  of  Effectiveness  on  Single  Eel 
EleUroplaques  of  Some  Synapse  Inactivating  and 
Synapse  Activating  Drugs 

Minimum  effective 
concentration 
Substance  '"  fg  per  ml 

a)  Compounds  which  inactivate  the  postsynaptic  membrane 
of  eel  electroplaques,  do  not  depolarize,  but  convert  the 
all-or-nothing    response    of    the    electrically    excitable 
membrane  to  the  gradedly  responsive 
Physostigmine  25 

</-Tubocurarine  50 

DFP*  100 

Procaine  200 

Tertiary  analog  of  prostigmine  1000 

Flaxedilt 

A)  Compounds  which  activate  synapses  of  eel  electroplaques. 
The  resultant  depolarization  secondarily  inactivates  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane.  Synaptic  electrogenesis 
still  occurs 

.\cetylcholinet  5 

Carbamylcholine  10 

Decamethonium  10 

Dimethylaminoethyl  acetate  (DMEA)t  50 

Prostigmine  5° 

SuccinylcholineK 

*  This  substance  causes  a  secondary  depolarization  with 
consequent  inactivation  of  the  electrically  excitable  mem- 
brane. 

t  Included  on  the  basis  of  the  data  of  Chagas  &  Albe- 
Fessard  (39)  that  the  action  of  Flaxedil  is  similar  to  that  of 
curare.  These  workers  did  not  study  membrane  potentials  or 
graded  responsiveness.  Chemically  Flaxedil  is  tri-(diethyl- 
aminoethoxy)  benzene  triethyliodide. 

I  In  the  presence  of  25  ^g  per  ml  physostigmine. 

Tl  On  the  basis  of  the  data  of  Chagas  &  Albe-Fessard 
C39).  who  found  a  similarity  of  action  with  acetylcholine 
(see  note  f). 


since  it  applies  as  well  10  hyperpolarizing  synapses 
and  to  systems  containing  both  electrogenic  types 
(96,  97).  The  two  major  varieties  of  drugs  are  in  this 
case  classified  as  activators  or  inacti\ators  of  synaptic 
electrogenesis.  The  nature  of  the  latter,  depolarizing 
or  hyperpolarizing,  is  determined  only  by  the  type 
of  synapse  not  by  the  activator  substance.  Each  major 
group  is  subdivided  into  drugs  which  act  nonselec- 
tively  or  selectively  upon  either  the  depolarizing  or 
hyperpolarizing  synapses.  The  interactions  of  drugs 
and  synapses  disclose  many  sui)sidiary  classifications, 
both  in  the  drugs  and  in  synaptic  membranes  (99, 
100,  108),  but  these  need  not  be  considered  here. 

The  overt  manifestations  of  'excitation'  and  'in- 
hibition' of  the  six  classes  of  drugs  in  table  3  need 
not  correspond  to  the  basic  mode  of  achieving  this 
effect  at  the  synaptic  level.  Thus,  the  'excitant'  ac- 


TABLE  3.   Possible  Combinations   of   Actions   of 
Synaptic  Drugs 


Effect 

Synapses  Affected 

Overt  .Action 

Agent 

Depola- 
rizing 

(Excita- 
tory) 

Hyper- 
polar- 
izing 

(Inhibi- 
tory) 

Type  Compound 

.Activators 

2 

3 

+ 
+ 
0 

+ 
0 

+ 

Excitation 
Excitation 
Inhibition 

Acetylcholine 
Metrazol 

Inacti\ators 

4 
5 
6 

+ 
+ 
0 

+ 
0 

+ 

Inhibition 
Inhibition 
Excitation 

Curare 
GABA 

Strychnine 

-f  indicates  an  effect;  o,  none.  Diphasic  actions  omitted. 


TABLE  4.  Cortical  Synaptic  Actions   of  Aliphatic 
Amino  Acids 


Car- 
bon 
No.    a-amino  acids 


w-ammo  acn 


ids 


Glycine 


3  o 

(a-alanine) 


)-diamino  acids 
X 

X 


(/3-alanine) 
(7-aminobutyric) 


(a-aminobu-  (7-aminobutyric)  (2,4diamino- 
tyric)  butyric) 

50  --  o 

(Norvaline)  (a-amino  ly-valeric)  (Ornithine) 
6                 o                                  +++  + 

(Norleucine)  (e-amino  caproic)  (Lysine) 
8               X                             -f-|-  +  +  X 

(co-amino  caprylic) 

Symbols:  —    to indicate  increasing  blockade  of 

excitatory  synapses  which  leads  to  overt  inhibitory'  action; 
+  to  -|-  +  -|--|-  represent  increasing  blockade  of  inhibitory 
synapses  leading  to  'excitatory'  effects;  o,  compound  not 
active;  X,  not  available  or  not  tried. 


tions  of  the  two  conxulsant  agents,  strychnine  and 
pentylentetrazol,  are  produced  by  entirely  difTerent 
fundamental  processes  (166).  The  similarities  in 
overt  cfl'ects  arise  from  the  conditions  that  prevail  in 
systems  which  contain  many  synapses  and  of  both 
tvpes.  It  is  then  likely  that  an  activity  is  a  mixture 
involving  both  excitatory  and  inhibitory  synaptic 
actions,  and  the  study  of  the  central  nervous  system 
has  revealed  many  examples  of  this.  Blockade  of 
synaptic  activity  thus  becomes  a  positive  act,  en- 
hancing or  diminishing  overt  manifestations  such  as 
motor    activity,    depending    upon    which    type    of 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


■77 


FIG.  22.  Synaptic  actions  of  shorter-chain  u-amino  acids. 
Column  A  shows  the  response  evoked  in  the  cat  cerebral  cortex 
by  a  local  electrical  stimulus  (five  superimposed  traces  indicate 
the  degree  of  variability).  The  surface  negative  potential  (up- 
ward deflection)  is  the  p.s.p.  of  superficial  dendrites.  B  shows 
the  effects  of  applying  0.2  cc  of  a  i  per  cent  buffered  w-amino 
acid.  The  substances  are  identified  by  the  letters  on  the  left 
which  correspond  to  those  in  table  4.  .'Ml  the  compounds  in- 
verted the  surface  negativity  to  a  surface  positi\'ity  by  blocking 
production  of  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  and  thereby  disclosing  hy- 
perpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  which  are  recorded  as  surface  positivity 
(downward  deflection).  The  action  of  all  four  substances  was 
similar  but  differed  in  magnitude  and  rate  of  onset,  both  factors 
being  largest  with  C,  (GAB.A).  Column  C  shows  that  recovery 
from  the  action  of  the  compounds  is  seen  3  min.  after  rinsing 
the  cortical  surface  several  times  with  Ringer's  solution.  Time 
at  bottom,  20  msec.  [From  Purpura  el  al.  (163).] 

synapse  is  inactivated.  Il  is  for  this  reason  that 
selecti\e  Ijlockade  of  inhibitory  synapses  by  strych- 
nine leads  to  excitatory'  actions,  augmented  elec- 
trical activity  or  convulsions. 

The  selective  action  of  many  drugs  on  either  hy- 
perpolarizing  or  depolarizing  synapses  introduces  an 
important  factor.  A  substance  may  act  powerfully 
on  one  synaptic  .system  and  yet  be  inert  with  respect 
to  another  which  lacks  the  appropriate  synaptic 
substrate  for  the  drug.  This  has  been  experimentally 
verified  with  strychnine  which  is  a  highly  selective 
inactivator  of  hyperpolarizing  inhibitory  synapses 
(fig.  14).  Strychnine  is  inert,  except  in  very  high 
concentrations,   on   structures   like   the   muscle   end- 


plate  or  the  vermian  cerebellar  cortex  of  cat.  How- 
ever, when  given  in  high  concentration  it  does  act 
to  blockade  the  depolarizing  excitatory  synapses 
C166). 

In  view  of  the  foregoing,  tests  on  relatively  simple 
synapses  (table  2;  figs.  11,  13,  15)  may  not  be  ade- 
quate for  analyzing  drug  actions.  This  fact  is  illus- 
trated by  the  recent  demonstration  and  analysis  of 
the  synaptic  actions  of  various  amino  acids  (162, 
163).  The  a)-amino  acids  tested  (table  4),  substances 
in  which  the  amino  group  is  on  the  terminal  carbon 
farthest  from  the  carboxyl  radical,  are  selective  in- 
activators  of  cortical  synapses.  The  shorter  chain 
compounds  (C2  to  C5,  fig.  22)  block  depolarizing 
activity  of  the  dendrites  while  compounds  Ce  and 
Cs  (fig.  23)  inactivate  hyperpolarizing  synapses. 

One  of  these  substances,  7-aminobutyric  acid 
(GABA),  occurs  naturally  in  the  brain  (12,  173)  and 
has  been  identified  (16)  as  a  component  of  the  'in- 
hibitory factor'  which  can  be  extracted  from  mam- 
malian brain  and  which  diminishes  the  discharge 
of  impulses  in  the  mechanically  excited  crayfish 
stretch  receptor.  As  a  selective  blockader  of  de- 
polarizing receptor  and  synaptic  membrane,  GABA 
can  only  act  as  an  ostensible  'inhibitor'  when  con- 
fronted with  the  simple  depolarizing  electrogenic 
membrane.  Thus  it  acts  on  the  cerebellar  cortex  as 


CAPROIC  (Cg) 


FIG.  23.  The  qualitatively  difl"erent  effects  produced  by 
w-amino  acids  with  longer  carbon  chains.  In  each  experiment, 
responses  were  simultaneously  evoked  from  the  surface  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  (upper  trace)  and  cerebellar  cortex  (lower 
trace).  /  and  4  show  the  responses  in  different  experiments  be- 
fore applying  the  amino  acids;  2  and  5,  the  cerebral  p.s.p.'s 
increased  on  applying  C^  or  Cg.  The  cerebellar  activity  was  not 
affected  indicating  that  these  u-amino  acids  are  inert  toward 
the  cerebellum.  3  and  6,  responses  after  rinsing  the  cortical 
surfaces  with  Ringer's  solution.  Time,  20  msec,  is  different  in 
the  two  experiments.  Four  traces  superimposed  in  each  record. 
[From  Purpura  et  al.  (162).] 


■78 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


it  does  on  crayfish  stretch  receptor  membrane  by 
eHminating  the  depolarizing  electrogenesis  (fig.  24). 
However,  when  GABA  is  applied  to  the  cerebral 
cortex,  its  selective  elimination  of  depolarizing 
surface-negative  p.s.p.'s  discloses  the  previously 
masked  hyperpolarizing  surface-positive  p.s.p.'s. 
Acting  in  the  cerebral  cortex  (figs.  22,  24)  GABA 
and  its  congeners  invert  the  electrocortical  activity 
evoked  by  a  stimulus. 

The  effects  of  the  .selective  inactivators  of  hyper- 
polarizing synapses,  Ce  and  Cg  (fig.  23),  also  differ 
depending  upon   the   type  of  electrogenic   structure 


20  MSEC 


FIG.  ^4.  Different  effects  of  the  selective  inactivator  of  de- 
polarizing p.s.p.'s  at  different  sites.  A,  t  to  5.-  Simultaneous  re- 
cordings from  the  cerebral  cortex  with  a  large  surface  electrode 
(upper  trace)  and  a  fine  wire  electrode  (lower  trace).  /,  both 
electrodes  were  on  the  surface  and  recorded  nearly  identically 
the  evoked  surface  negative  p.s.p.'s  of  the  superficial  cerebral 
dendrites.  2,  the  fine  electrode  was  inserted  about  0.4  mm  be- 
low the  surface  into  an  essentially  isoelectric  region.  3  and  4, 
application  of  GABA  to  the  cortical  surface  reversed  the  surface 
response  into  positivity,  but  this  change  did  not  appear  in  the 
subsurface  recording.  This  indicates  that  the  effect  produced  by 
the  amino  acid  was  on  superficial  p.s.p.'s  only,  j,  rinsing  the 
cortical  surface  restored  the  original  activity  at  the  surface.  The 
subsurface  recording  was  still  unchanged.  6,  superimposed 
responses  before  and  during  the  action  of  G.^B.'K.  B:  The  simul- 
taneous recordings  in  this  experiment  were  from  the  cerebral 
cortex  (upper  trace)  and  the  cerebellar  (lower  trace).  /,  before 
applying  GABA;  2,  five  drops  of  0.1  per  cent  G,'\B.'\  were  ap- 
plied to  each  site.  In  the  cerebral  cortex  the  result  was  a  reversal 
of  surface  potential.  In  the  cerebellar  cortex  the  surface  nega- 
tivity was  eliminated  by  blockade  of  the  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s, 
but  no  positivity  developed  because  of  the  paucity  of  hyper- 
polarizing synapses  in  this  structure.  3,  recovery  was  rapid  in 
the  cerebral  and  slower  in  the  cerebellar  cortex.  Time,  20  msec. 
[From  Purpura  et  al.  (163).] 


that  is  used  as  a  test  object.  Neither  the  crayfish 
stretch  receptor  nor  the  cerebellar  cortex  is  affected 
by  application  of  co-aminocaprylic  acid  (Cj).  How- 
ever, the  surface  negativity  evoked  in  the  cerebral 
cortex  is  augmented  by  the  blockade  which  Ce  and 
Cs  cause  amongst  the  surface-positive  p.s.p.'s  of  the 
hyperpolarizing  synapses. 

Recent  work  (Grundfest  et  al.,  in  preparation;  cf. 
99,  163)  indicates  that  the  axodendritic  synaptic 
membrane  in  the  cat  brain  stands  in  a  doubly  in- 
verted pharmacological  relation  with  some  crusta- 
cean synapses.  GABA  and  other  inactivators  of  the 
cat  depolarizing  synapses  activate  crustacean  in- 
hibitory synapses.  Picrotoxin,  an  activator  of  cat 
excitatory  synapses,  inactivates  the  crustacean 
inhibitory  synapses.  One  of  the  selective  inactivators 
of  cat  inhibitory  synapses,  carnitine  (cf.  163),  activates 
the  excitatory  synapses  of  lobster  muscle  fibers. 
However,  these  inverted  parallels  are  not  complete. 
The  Ce  and  Cs  co-amino  acids  do  not  affect  the 
crustacean  synapses.  Likewise,  acetylcholine,  d-luho- 
curarine  and  strychnine  are  without  effect. 

In  sum,  it  may  be  concluded  from  the  foregoing 
discussion  that  determination  of  the  mode  of  action 
of  a  drug  depends  not  only  on  the  degree  of  intimate 
knowledge  which  may  be  obtained  of  its  synaptic 
effects  but  also  upon  the  type  of  information  that 
may  be  provided  by  the  test  object.  The  synaptic 
structure  u.sed  for  the  tests  may  be  too  complex  to 
yield  the  details  required,  but  also  it  may  be  too 
simple  and  provide  only  misleadingly  partial  in- 
formation. 

Identification   and  Characterization   oj   Transmitter   Agents 

The  preceding  section  sets  the  theoretical  and 
methodological  background  for  the  problems  treated 
in  this.  The  quantity  of  transmitters  released  during 
activity  of  presynaptic  terininals  is  probably  ex- 
ceedingly small  (cf.  52,  59,  60,  68).  The  problem  of 
their  identification  therefore  is  strongly  conditioned 
by  methodology.  For  example,  norepinephrine  has 
been  known,  since  its  laboratory  synthesis  in  1904 
(cf.  193),  to  have  properties  similar  to  those  of  its 
homologue,  epinephrine.  Also,  the  work  of  Cannon 
and  his  associates  (cf.  1 77)  had  indicated  very 
clearly  that  there  must  be  at  least  several  sym- 
pathetic transmitters  which  were  designated  as 
sympathins  E  (excitatory)  and  I  (inhibitory).  Never- 
theless, norepinephrine  was  not  accepted  as  a  pos- 
sible sympathetic  transmitter  until  it  was  shown  in 
1946  (cf   193)  that  it  is  a  natural  constitutent  of  the 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


'79 


body.  Likewise,  interest  in  GABA  stems  from  the 
demonstration  of  its  occurrence  in  the  brain  in  an 
important  pathway  of  synthesis  (12,  172).  Thus,  the 
candidate  for  a  transmitter  agent  must  meet  a  num- 
ber of  requirements  (cf.  also  65):  a)  it  must  mimic 
closely  the  actions  produced  by  the  natural,  neural 
stimulus;  b')  its  actions  must  be  affected  by  the  same 
drugs  and  in  the  same  ways  as  neural  excitation  is 
modified;  c)  it  must  be  a  naturally  occurring  con- 
stitutent,  found  in  close  proximity  to  the  relevant 
synaptic  structures;  and  (/)  it  is  desirable  to  demon- 
strate that  it  is  formed  by  an  appropriate  metabolic 
pathway,  that  it  is  released  at  the  time,  place  and  in 
the  degree  suitable  to  transmitter  action  and  that  its 
accumulation  to  excess  is  prevented  by  another 
metabolic  pathway. 

Characterized  by  the  foregoing  criteria,  acetyl- 
choline and  the  catechol  amines  of  the  epinephrine 
group  are  still  the  only  substances  commonly  agreed 
upon  and  accepted  as  peripheral  transmitter  agents. 
Most  conspicuously,  these  substances  derive  their 
claim  to  transmitter  agents  by  their  actions  as 
synapse  activators.  Thus,  acetylcholine  is  probably 
the  excitatory  transmitter  at  electroplaques,  muscle 
fibers,  autonomic  ganglia  and  some  gland  cells.  At 
the  effector  junctions  of  the  cardiac  pacemaker  and 
probably  also  in  many  smooth  muscle  systems  (96), 
acetylcholine  activates  hyperpolarizing  synapses  and 
is  inhibitory.  The  epinephrine  group  of  transmitters 
acts  similarly  at  other  synapses.  However,  these 
transmitters  also  appear  to  have  an  accessory  func- 
tion (cf.  36).  Thus  epinephrine  may  antagonize  the 
action  of  decamethonium  (47)  or  relieve  '  fatigue'  of 
neuromuscular  transmission  upon  repetitive  stimula- 
tion (119).^ 

In  complex  synaptic  systems,  one  may  as.sign 
transmitter  action  to  substances  which  do  inac- 
tivate synapses.  For  example,  GABA  is  a  synapse  in- 
activator,  but  if  it  is  released  by  specific  nerve  fibers 
its  effects  would  be  essentially  inhibitory — with  the 
important  exception  that  there  would  be  no  accom- 
paniment of  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.  Likewise  there 
might  be  transmitters,  analogous  to  Cg,  whose  overt 

^  Neuromuscular  blockade  by  decamethonium  is  a  manifes- 
tation of  Wedensky  inhibition  discussed  earlier.  Antagonism  by 
epinephrine  suggests  that  this  transmitter  agent  acts  as  a  com- 
petitive antagonist,  or  synapse  inactivator,  of  cholinoceptive 
synaptic  membrane.  This  type  of  action  is  apparently  contra- 
dicted by  the  ciTect  of  epinephrine  in  lifting  the  blockade  pro- 
duced by  repetitive  activity.  However,  there  need  be  no  real 
contradiction  for  synaptic  membrane  may  change  its  properties 
under  different  experimental  circumstances,  an  indication  of 
the  complexity  as  well  as  lability  of  the  active  structure  (cf.  96). 


action,  excitation,  might  be  produced  by  inactivat- 
ing hyperpolarizing  inhibitory  synapses. 

These  considerations  indicate  the  difficulty  of 
identifying  transmitters  in  a  complexly  organized 
synaptic  structure.  The  difficulty  is  enormously  com- 
pounded in  the  central  nervous  system,  where  even 
a  small  volume  of  tissue  contains  a  huge  number  of 
synapses.  In  such  a  case  all  the  criteria  for  categoriz- 
ing transmitters  cannot  be  fulfilled  at  present  and 
therefore  identification  is  always  tentative,  based  as 
it  must  be  on  incomplete  evidence. 

Nevertheless,  there  is  evidence  from  various  sources 
that  acetylcholine  and  the  adrenergic  agents  do  af- 
fect central  nervous  activity.  Thus,  circulatory  injec- 
tions of  epinephrine  (22)  or  acetylcholine  (cf.  iii) 
bring  about  EEG  activation  as  does  stimulation  of 
the  peripheral  stump  of  the  cat  splanchnic  nerve 
(22).  The  electrical  activity  of  a  cortical  slab,  iso- 
lated from  its  neural  connections  but  surviving  with 
intact  blood  supply,  is  altered  upon  electrical  stimu- 
lation of  the  brain  stem  reticular  formation  (122). 
Thus,  brain  stem  activity  releases  some  transmitter 
agents  which  can  then  affect  the  isolated  cortex. 
This  finding  has  been  extended  to  cross-perfused 
preparations  (160).  From  that  work  it  may  be  con- 
cluded that  what  is  released  during  brain  stem  ac- 
tivity enters  the  systemic  circulation  and  that  it 
must  be  a  substance  (or  several)  more  stable  than  is 
acetylcholine.  The  latter  probably  would  have  been 
destroyed  completely  or  almost  so  during  the  time 
required  for  an  exchange  of  circulating  blood  be- 
tween donor  and  host.  Many  workers  have  shown 
that  acetylcholine  is  found  in  the  central  nervous 
system  as  well  as  its  synthesizing  acetylating  enzyme 
(for  references  to  the  recent  literature  cf.  65).  The 
distributions  of  these  substances  in  the  brain  and  of 
sympathetic  transmitters  (192)  have  also  been 
mapped.  Lesions  in  some  regions  of  the  reticular 
formation  augment  or  depress  the  sensitivity  of  the 
cortical  electrical  activity  to  epinephrine  (178) 
Intraventricular  application  of  cholinomimetic  and 
adrenomimetic  substances  or  of  blockaders  of  the 
two  types  of  synapses  produce  a  variety  of  central 
nervous  symptoms  (cf.  75,  and  literature  cited  there). 
Intravenous  injections  of  (/-tubocurarine  block  cen- 
tral nervous  synapses  (cf.  165). 

Evidence  with  respect  to  other  agents  is  still  in- 
conclusive. Although  5-hydroxytryptamine  (sero- 
tonin) and  metabolically  related  substances  are  be- 
lieved by  some  to  be  implicated  in  transmission, 
whether  they  act  directly  or  not  is  still  in  question 
(cf.   26,  128,    148).   As  stressed  earlier  in  this  part. 


i8o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  difficulties  are  largely  methodological  because 
central  synapses  are  so  intricately  interrelated  and 
may  present  many  varieties.  Stemming  from  this  is 
the  difficulty  of  determining  whether  or  not  various 
synaptic  sites  are  afifected,  and  if  they  are  what 
results  are  to  be  looked  for.  Thus  it  has  been  shown 
(162,  163)  that  the  synaptically  active  amino  acids 
affect  primarily  only  axodendritic  synapses  of  the 
cortex  and  only  secondarily  the  axosomatic.  Some 
consecjuences  are  seen  in  figure  25.  Blockade  of  ex- 
citatory synapses  of  the  dendrites  by  C4  (GABA)  or 
of  inhibitory  synapses  by  Ce  (e-aminocaproic  acid) 
does  not  afTect  the  corticospinal  discharge  of  the 
pyramidal  cells.  However,  the  convulsive  electro- 
cortical  activity  induced  by  Cs  (oj-aminocaprylic 
acid)  leads  to  prolonged  discharge  in  the  tract. 

A  further  difficulty  is  the  problem  of  accessibility 
of  the  central  synapses  to  testing  drugs.  The  blood- 
brain  barrier  apparently  is  highly  effective  for  some 


FIG.  25.  Pyramidal  tract  activity  when  dendritic  responses 
in  cerebral  cortex  are  affected  by  u-amino  acids.  Column  A,  the 
discharge  recorded  from  the  pyramidal  tracts  to  stimulation  of 
the  cerebral  cortex  in  cats.  Then,  0.2  cc  of  i  per  cent  u-amino 
acid  had  been  applied  for  10  min.  The  substances  were  C4  and 
Cs  as  noted  in  each  row  of  records.  The  responses  of  column  B 
were  obtained  when  the  cortical  potentials  had  been  altered  as 
shown  in  figs.  22  and  24.  Despite  these  changes,  the  pyramidal 
tract  responses,  generated  by  direct  electrical  stimuli  and  by 
axosomatic  synaptic  excitations,  were  not  affected  (C4,  Ce) 
except  when  as  in  the  case  of  C^,  the  drug  caused  convulsions. 
Then  a  long  after-dischsirge,  associated  with  the  convulsions, 
developed.  Ten  superimposed  traces  in  the  upper  records,  five 
in  the  middle  and  lower  set.  Time  10  msec.  [From  Purpura  et 
a/.  (163).] 


substances,  e.g.  GABA  [Roberts  &  Baxter  (172)]. 
Recent  experiments  (164)  demonstrate  that  local 
abolition  of  the  blood-brain  barrier  permits  the  local 
action  of  systemically  injected  oj-amino  acids.  These 
results  indicate  that  if  the  substances  are  elaborated 
within  the  brain  they  might  act  as  transmitters  (using 
the  term  for  both  synapse  activators  and  inactivators; 
cf.  above),  although  the  usual  experimental  criteria 
would  not  disclose  such  action. 

Modes  of  Action  of  Transmitter  Agents  and  Synaptic  Drugs 

Since  transmitters  must  be  formed  and,  after  their 
release,  metabolized  in  the  body,  enzymes  for  these 
activities  are  components  more  or  less  related  to  the 
appropriate  synaptic  systems.  In  the  search  for 
mechanisms  of  drug  action,  interference  with  en- 
zymatic or  other  metabolic  processes  has  been  fre- 
quently stressed  (cf.  2,  13,  14,  26,  65,  and  literature 
cited  in  these  papers).  Undoubtedly-,  interference 
with  these  metabolic  systems  must  cause  synaptic 
disturbance;  but  it  is  likely  that  such  actions  are 
relatively  slow,  manifesting  themselves,  as  in  the 
case  of  vitamin  deficiences,  only  after  depletion  of 
reserves.  This  is  not  the  case  with  drugs  that  have 
primary  action  on  synapses  (108).  This  may  be  .seen 
in  figures  22  to  24  in  which  the  synaptic  effects  of 
some  of  the  oj-amino  acids  were  obtained  within  a 
second  after  they  were  applied  and  were  rapidly 
reversed  by  dilution. 

Furthermore,  substances  of  the  same  type  of  action 
on  enzymatic  systems  may  have  entirely  different 
synaptic  actions.  Thus  physostigmine,  DFP  and 
prostigmine  are  powerful  inhibitors  of  cholinesterase. 
By  that  effect  all  three,  in  very  high  dilution,  enhance 
neural  action  in  eel  electroplaques  (5).  This  is  merely 
the  indication  that  they  prolong  the  life  of  the  labile 
transmitter  agent.  However,  the  synaptic  actions  of 
the  three  drugs  on  the  eel  electroplaque  are  diverse 
(table  2).  In  that  capacity  prostigmine  is  a  synapse 
activator  like  acetylcholine  itself  Physostigmine  js  an 
inactivator  like  (/-tubocurarine  and  about  as  potent 
in  that  effect.  DFP  appears  to  have  dual  actions 
such  as  are  to  be  found  in  the  many  other  situations 
(cf.  95,  96,  127). 

The  conclusion  reached  from  these  considerations 
leads  back  to  the  view  first  proposed  by  Ehrlich  (cf 
2,  14,  40)  that  drugs  exert  their  action  by  affecting, 
perhaps  by  some  form  of  chemical  or  electrostatic 
combination,  the  performance  of  specific  molecular 
structures  of  the  cell  membrane.  This  receptor  theory 
has  had  many  vicissitudes,  apparently  largely  be- 
cause of  static  conceptions  of  such  functional  units. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


lai 


Recently  the  models  examined  have  been  endowed 
with  dynamic  properties  (cf.  2,  9,  and  literature 
cited  in  the  papers).  These  current  theoretical 
formulations  have  had  some  success  in  accounting 
for  relations  between  structures  of  drugs  and  their 
functions.  They  do  not,  as  yet,  consider  the  implica- 
tions of  the  recent  findings  concerning  specificity  of 
drug  action  on  one  or  the  other  type  of  synaptic 
membrane.  Thus  the  addition  of  one  carbon  link  to 
an  co-amino  acid  converts  a  substance  which  is  pre- 
dominantly an  inactivator  of  depolarizing  synapses 
(Cs)  to  another  (Ce)  which  inactivates  chiefly,  or 
perhaps  exclusively,  the  hyperpolarizing  type  (table 
4;  figs.  22,  23).  In  other  relations  of  drugs,  similar 
abrupt  transitions  depending  upon  number  of 
carbons  (the  transformation  occurring  at  about  five 
carbon.s)  have  also  been  noted  (cf  14,  p.   147). 

The  occurrence  of  distinct  varieties  of  synaptically 
acting  chcmotherapeutic  agents,  e.g.  analgesics, 
antipyretics,  etc.,  bespeaks  relatively  sharp,  though 
not  absolute,  difl^erences  between  synaptic  mem- 
branes in  differently  acting  regions  of  the  central 
nervous  system.  Similar  distinctions,  both  peripheral 
and  central,  derive  from  the  relatively  specific  actions 
of  other  drugs.  Thus,  whether  synaptic  transmission 
is  blockaded  by  atropine  or  by  fZ-tubocurarine  forms 
part  of  the  differentiation  between  muscarinic  and 
nicotinic  cholinoceptive  synapses. 

Ph ysio/og ical  Im plica lio n s 

Only  a  few  selected  aspects  can  be  discussed  here 
of  the  relations  between  the  modes  of  action  of  trans- 
mitter agents  and   their  physiological  consequences. 

a)  topographic  distinctions.  In  many  cases  the 
action  of  a  transmitter  must  be  rather  strictly  lo- 
calized. This  is  due  to  a  number  of  factors  which 
differ  in  importance  for  diflferent  transmitters  and 
synaptic  sites.  The  small  quantity  of  transmitter  re- 
leased by  a  presynaptic  nerve  fiber  would  rapidly 
lose  effectiveness  upon  diffu.sion  and  dilution  in  the 
volume  away  from  the  synaptic  site.  It  may  be 
destroyed  by  enzymes  or  fLxed  in  various  chemical 
combinations.  Its  effectiveness  at  other  synaptic 
sites  may  be  small  or  absent.  The  rate  at  which  it 
moves  from  the  region  in  which  it  was  liberated  may 
be  very  slow. 

These,  and  other  factors  that  may  be  postulated, 
tend  to  restrict  transmitter  action  to  limited  sites, 
although  under  special  experimental  conditions  dif- 
fusion is  easily  demonstrated  (cf  177).  Electrical 
ine.xcitability  of  .synaptic  membrane  and  its  chemical 


specificities  promote  restriction  of  transmitter  action 
which  is  desirable  in  intricate  synaptic  relations.  The 
specificities  of  different,  perhaps  of  alternating 
synapses  in  a  .synaptic  sequence,  as  suggested  by 
Feldberg  (cf  157),  would  be  one  means  of  achieving 
this  result.  In  the  spinal  cord,  interneurons  and 
motoneurons  appear  to  have  somewhat  different 
pharmacological  properties  (cf.  60). 

Diff'erent  parts  of  the  same  neuron  might  also  have 
differently  sensitive  synaptic  membranes.  Thus,  the 
co-amino  acids  appear  to  be  chiefly  effective  as 
synapse  inactivators  at  the  superificial  axodendritic 
synapses  of  cortical  neurons  (fig.  25).  In  the  context 
of  electrically  inexcitable  activity  of  these  dendrites 
(165)  the  function  of  dendritic  electrogenesis  is  prob- 
ably that  of  modulating  somatic  responsiveness,  a 
consequence    which    cannot    be   discussed    here   (cf. 

lOl). 

b)  synaptic  specificity  and  transmitters.  Eccles 
(cf.  60)  has  emphasized  the  implication  of  Dale's 
suggestion  (46)  that  one  neuron  at  all  its  profuse 
terminals  probably  generates  only  one  type  of  trans- 
mitter. This  '  principle'  is  reasonable  but  is  not  at 
all  an  obligatory  condition.  Furthermore,  a  neuron 
secreting  the  same  transmitter  at  different  synaptic 
sites  may  produce  depolarization  and  be  an  'ex- 
citant' at  one,  or  cause  hyperpolarization  and  be  an 
'inhibitor'  at  another  variety  of  synaptic  membrane. 
Likewise,  the  same  neuron  might  produce  at  its 
different  terminals  several  varieties  of  transmitters 
which  might  all  have  the  same  effect,  excitatory  or 
inhibitory,  or  opposite  actions,  depending  entirely 
upon  the  variety  of  postsynaptic  membrane  which  is 
in  synaptic  relation  with  the  transmitters.  This 
emphasizes  that  the  nature  of  the  transmitter  can 
determine  synaptic  potency  and  the  kinetics  of  the 
synaptic  activity  (cf  97).  The  type  of  electrogenic 
action  is  determined  by  the  postsynaptic  membrane.' 

c)  reciprocal  interactions  of  neural  pathways. 
The  mechanisms  of  dual  action  discussed  above  have 
bearing  upon  the  interpretation  of  reciprocal  innerva- 
tion.r  Sherington  discovered  in  spinal  reflexes  (44) 
that  the  development  of  reflex  activity  in  one  muscle 
is  associated  with  concurrent  inhibition  of  antagonis- 
tic muscular  activity.   These  interactions  extend   to 

'  Interactions  of  some  drugs  evoke  apparently  dual  actions 
at  the  muscle  endplatc  (cf.  53).  These  may  be  cases  of  the  situa- 
tion commented  upon  earlier,  in  which  a  drug  activates  some 
components  and  inactivates  others  in  the  same  synaptic  mem- 
brane. This  implies  that  the  membrane  of  a  single  synapse  is 
not  homogeneous. 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


Other  muscle  groups  that  participate  in  an  organized 
movement  (cf.  140).  These  effects  frequently  are 
fully  reciprocal,  excitation  in  either  path  being  asso- 
ciated with  inhibition  in  the  other,  and  thus  they 
involve  processes  of  reciprocal  inhibition  as  well  as 
primary  excitation. 

A  mechanism,  discovered  by  Lloyd  (cf.  138)  and 
termed  direct  inhibition,  was  believed  to  be  mediated 
monosynaptically,  the  collaterals  of  the  same  afferent 
nerve  fiber  at  one  motoneuron  evoking  excitation, 
tho.se  to  another  producing  inhibition.  This  mecha- 
nism would  imply  that  the  same  transmitter  evokes 
depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  in  one  neuron  and  hyperpolariz- 
ing  p.s.p.'s  in  another  cell.  From  the  point  of  view  of 
theoretical  considerations  this  means  of  achieving 
reciprocal  actions  is  perfectly  feasible,  as  is  the  possi- 
bilitv  that  different  transmitters  are  released  at  the 
different  terminals  of  the  same  primary  afferent  nerve 
fiber.  In  the  lobster  cardiac  ganglion  (33,  186)  one 
presynaptic  nerve  causes  excitation  in  one  neuron 
and  inhibition  in  another. 

The  reality  of  monosynaptic  direct  inhibition  of 
this  type  in  the  cat  spinal  cord  is  at  present  in  dis- 
pute. Lloyd  and  his  colleagues  (cf.  194)  maintain 
that  a  monosynaptic  pathway  exists  while  Eccles 
and  his  associates  (cf.  60)  consider  that  'direct'  in- 
hibition is  a  disynaptic  event.  Whether  the  particu- 
lar reflex  pathways  under  discussion  are  mono- 
synaptic or  disynaptic  is  probably  a  matter  of  the 
specific  structures  involved  and  perhaps  of  the 
methodological  details.*  In  principle,  direct  inhibi- 
tion by  monosynaptic  reciprocal  innervation  can 
occur.  Since  it  is  theoretically  feasible  it  seems  un- 
likely that  among  the  many  types  of  connections 
elaborated  in  the  nervous  system  one  possible  and 
rather  simple  variety  has  been  omitted. 


ROLE     OF    ELEMENT.-^RY  SYN.\PTIC  PROPERTIES 
IN    INTEGRATIVE    ACTIVITY 

Spatial  Interrelations  of  Synaptic  and 
Conductile  Aiembrane 

Since  p.s.p.'s  are  'standing'  nonpropagated  po- 
tentials,  their  effect  upon   the  electrically  excitable 

"  Drugs  such  as  pentobarbital,  for  example,  can  alter  pro- 
foundly the  pathways  that  produce  pyramidal  tract  activity 
through  thalamocortical  relays.  This  is  disclosed  by  changes  in 
latency  of  several  msec,  when  4  to  10  mg  per  kg  of  pentobarbital 
are  administered  (99,  loi,  161  and  unpublished  work). 


membrane  of  the  same  cell  depends  upon  the  spatial 
arrangement  of  these  differently  excitable  structures. 
Assuming  as  a  first  approximation  that  the  elec- 
trically excitable  membrane  everywhere  in  a  cell  is 
triggered  to  discharge  a  spike  by  the  same  level  of 
critical  depolarization,  and  that  the  depolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  are  everywhere  equal  in  amplitude,  the  in- 
tensity of  excitation  of  the  former  by  the  latter  will 
depend  upon  the  distance  between  the  synaptic 
focus  and  the  nearest  conductile  membrane.  The 
more  closely  the  two  electrogenic  membrane  sites 
approximate  each  other  the  more  intense  will  be  the 
excitation  for  triggering  a  spike.  The  apical  dendrites 
of  the  cerebral  cortex  are  not  electrically  excitable 
(107)  and  the  p.s.p.'s  of  the  axodendritic  synapses 
generated  at  some  distance  from  electrically  ex- 
citable membrane  therefore  would  not  be  expected 
to  be  as  effective  as  the  axosomatic  p.s.p.'s  generated 
in  close  contiguity  with  electrically  excitable  mem- 
brane. Thus,  the  apical  dendrites  of  cortical  neurons, 
although  they  generate  intense  synaptic  activity 
(165)  are  not  as  effective  in  triggering  spikes  as  are 
the  depolarizing  synaptic  loci  at  the  soma  (27,  loi). 
Spatial  considerations  may  also  be  applied  to  the 
effects  of  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s.  The  latter  would 
be  most  intensely  inhibitory  if  they  are  interposed 
between  sites  of  excitatory  p.s.p.'s  and  electrically 
excitable  membrane.  The  depolarizing  p.s.p.,  in 
that  case,  would  be  diminished  not  only  by  elec- 
trotonic  averaging  between  the  opposed  electro- 
genic actions.  The  interposed  hyperpolarizing  site 
would  receive  more  outward  current  flow  than  rest- 
ing membrane  since  its  resistance  would  be  lower, 
and  the  potential  gradient  steeper.  Consequently 
this  bypass  would  result  in  less  current  flow  from 
the  depolarizing  synaptic  site  to  the  electrically 
excitable  but  as  yet  inactive  membrane.  Thus,  the 
loci  at  which  depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  are  generated,  both  relati\e  to  each  other 
and  to  electrically  excitable  membrane,  must  play 
an  important  role  in  determining  the  effectiveness 
of  transmission  from  a  given  afferent  volley.  The 
simplifying  assumption  that  all  s\naptic  sites  are 
electrogenically  equivalent  is  probably  not  justified 
(see  below).  It  is  also  likely  that  the  conductile 
membrane  in  different  parts  of  a  cell  varies  with 
respect  to  its  electrical  threshold  (79)  or  that  it  is 
differently  electrogenic  (69),  and  these  factors  may 
reinforce  the  transmissional  inhomogeneity  of  dif- 
ferent synaptic  sites. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


183 


Physiological  Factors  Determining 
Transmissional  Effectiveness 

a)  SYNAPTIC  POTENCY  AND  DRIVE.  Just  as  different 
parts  of  the  same  cell  exhibit  variations  with  respect 
to  electrical  threshold  so  also  do  different  cells  in  a 
population,  that  is,  the  critical  firing  level  may  be 
lower  for  one  cell  than  for  another.  In  that  case,  an 
afferent  volley  equally  effective  in  generating  p.s.p.'s 
in  all  the  cells  may  discharge  some  of  these  but  not 
others.  It  is  unlikely,  however,  that  the  synaptic 
potency  of  a  given  influx  is  identical  for  all  cells. 
Even  in  single  multiply-innervated  cells,  such  as  the 
eel  electroplaque  (4),  the  p.s.p.'s  generated  over  the 
large  surface  are  of  different  amplitudes  and  always 
largest  at  a  definite  region  of  the  cell  surface.  Thus, 
the  p.s.p.'s  generated  in  a  population  of  motoneurons 
would  vary  in  magnitude  depending  upon  the 
synaptic  potency  of  the  afferents  to  each  cell.  This 
variation,  added  to  that  of  the  distribution  of  elec- 
trical thresholds,  results  in  a  population  spread  with 
respect  to  excitatory  effects  or  synaptic  drives.  It  is 
ob\'ious  that  the  degree  to  which  the  given  synaptic 
inflow  also  excites  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  as  well  as 
depolarizing,  and  the  relative  spatial  distributions  of 
the  two  electrogenic  activities  will  affect  the  mag- 
nitude of  the  synaptic  drive. 


The  differences  in  synaptic  drive  deduced  above 
adequately  account  for  a  mass  of  experimental  data. 
The  cells  in  a  population  of  neurons  impinged  upon 
by  a  sample  from  a  population  of  innervating  nerve 
fibers  will  respond  with  different  degrees  of  depolariz- 
ing p.s.p.'s.  Some  of  the  cells  will  discharge  spikes 
which  can  be  recorded  directly  (e.g.  fig.  12)  or  by 
means  of  other  effects,  as  for  example  by  their  reflex 
activation  of  muscle  in  the  case  of  motoneurons.  In 
other  cells  excited  by  the  afferent  volley  the  p.s.p.'s 
alone  are  generated. 

b)  EXCITED  AND  DISCHARGED  ZONES.  Thus,  an  ex- 
citatory volley  causing  quantitatively  different 
amounts  of  synaptic  activity  also  divides  the  popula- 
tion of  postjunctional  cells  qualitatively.  One  group, 
frequently  by  far  the  smaller,  falls  in  the  discharged 
zone,  the  other  in  the  excited  zone  (fig.  26).  In  this 
distribution  the  occurrence  and  influence  of  hyper- 
polarizing inhibitory  p.s.p.'s  may  also  be  considerable 
but  need  not  be  discussed  in  detail,  except  in  the 
extreme  case  when  the  neural  volley  generates  pre- 
dominantly or  entirely  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s.  In 
that  case  spike  electrogenesis  would  not  occur  and 
the  volley  in  isolation  may  produce  no  overt  effects, 
although  direct  recording  from  the  cells  would  dis- 


INNERVATED   FACE 


EXCITED    CELLS 
DISCHARGED    CELLS 


LEAD    A 


LEAD    B 

FIG.  26.  Discharged  and  excited  zones  in  a  row  of  eel  electroplaques  on  maximal  stimulation  of 
their  three  different  nerve  supplies.  Cells  6  to  10  were  excited  by  Nerve  I  as  evidenced  by  their  p.s.p.'s 
and  long-lasting  homosynaptic  facilitation,  but  did  not  develop  spikes  to  a  single  testing  stimulus. 
Nerve  II  caused  discharge  of  spikes  in  cells  9  to  11,  but  in  addition  excited  cells  6  to  8  and  12  and 
13.  Nerve  III  discharged  cells  12  and  13,  exciting  also  cells  10,  11,  14  and  15.  The  diagrammatic 
representation  of  the  recording  leads  shows  the  method  that  was  used  to  test  this  population  of  cells. 
[From  .Altamirano  et  at.  (5).] 


1 84 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


close  the  p.s.p.'s.  These,  hke  the  excitatory  p.s.p.'s, 
should  vary  in  amplitude  in  the  population  of  post- 
junctional cells.  An  overt  manifestation  of  the  in- 
hibitory p.s.p.'s  would  occur  if  the  cells  are  at  the 
same  time  producing  excitatory  p.s.p.'s  and  spikes  or 
causing  reflex  activity  in  muscles.  More  or  less  selec- 
tive afferent  activation  of  inhibition,  and  its  role  in 
spinal  reflex  activity,  was  demonstrated  by  Sherring- 
ton (44,  182;  cf.  140).  Relatively  specific  descending 
pathways  were  found  by  Sechenov  (180)  in  the  frog 
and  by  Magoun  and  his  colleagues  (cf.  147)  in  the 
mammal. 

c)  FACILITATION.  Study  of  spinal  cord  reflexes  also 
demonstrated  the  existence  of  the  excited  zone  by 
the  effects  of  temporal  and  spatial  facilitation,  the 
excited  cells  being  then  termed  the  subliminal  fringe. 
Both  types  of  facilitation  depend  essentially  upon 
the  properties  of  summation  and  sustained  response 


of  p.s.p.'s  described  above.  However,  subsidiary 
effects  also  participate  which  will  be  discussed  later. 
The  unitary  p.s.p.'s  are  relatively  long  lasting,  in 
the  cat  central  nervous  system  having  a  duration  of 
about  15  msec.  (figs.  12,  27).  For  that  time  at  least, 
therefore,  an  excited  cell  is  somewhat  depolarized,  at 
first  to  a  large  degree,  but  not  to  that  of  the  critical 
level  for  discharge,  and  then  to  a  smaller  amount, 
decreasing  with  time. 

The  presence  of  the  e.xcited  cells  can  be  tested  by 
applying  a  second  volley  either  through  the  pathway 
which  delivered  the  first  condidoning  stimulus 
(homosynaptic  testing)  or  through  another  inner\-at- 
ing  path  (hcterosynaptic  testing). 

d)  HOMOSYNAPTIC  FACILITATION.  In  this  casc,  there 
will  be  no  second  response,  neither  an  electrical 
activity  nor  a  reflex  contraction  of  muscles,  if  the 
stimulus  interval  is  verv  short.   Because  of  absolute 


B 


msec 


50  mV 


10  msec 


^ — ^^^ 


FIG.  27.  Temporal  facilitation  and  shortening  of  synaptic  delays  in  neurons.  .-1,  B:  From  a  cat 
motoneuron  at  high  and  low  amplification.  Two  orthodromic  stimuli,  neither  capable  of  discharging 
the  cell,  can  evoke  a  spike  by  summation  of  the  p.s.p.'s  produced  by  each  stimulus.  Since  the  spike 
occurs  only  when  the  critical  level  of  depolarization  is  attained,  the  summation  interval  may  be 
sharply  delineated  as  shown  in  this  example.  [From  Brock  el  al.  (24).]  C:  From  a  rabbit  cervical 
sympathetic  neuron.  Progressively  stronger  stimuli  to  the  preganglionic  nerve  increased  the  p.s.p. 
of  the  neuron  and  evoked  its  spike  earlier  as  the  critical  firing  level  (shown  by  arrows)  was  attained 
earlier.  [From  Eccles  (64).] 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


l8n 


% 

zoo 

""■        300 
Z50 

\ 

\ 

B 

150 

\» 

\ 

^v. 

■~. 

200 
150 

- 

\ 

V 

.^ 

2 

4 

6 

a 

10 

12  Msec  ( 

3 

2 

4 

6 

8 

10 

12  Msec. 

FIG.  28.  The  time  courses  of  facilita- 
tion and  direct  inhibition  (magnitude  of 
response  as  percent  of  control  value) 
tested  on  monosynaptic  reflexes  are 
nearly  symmetrical.  Facilitation  (upper 
curves^,  A  in  an  extensor  and  B  in  a  flexor 
muscle.  Inhibition  (lower  curves'),  i4  in  a 
flexor  and  B  in  an  extensor  muscle. 
[From  Lloyd  (140).] 


e       10      12Msec  0        2       4 


12  Msec. 


refractoriness  of  the  presynaptic  fibers,  no  impulses 
arrive  at  the  synapses.  At  slightly  longer  intervals, 
relative  refractoriness  or  persistent  absolute  refrac- 
toriness of  the  previously  discharged  postjunctional 
cells  causes  a  depressed  testing  response,  but  then  an 
interval  is  reached  when  the  testing  response  can 
become  many  times  higher  than  it  would  have  been 
without  the  preceding  conditioning  activity.  As  noted 
above,  the  facilitation  in  the  simplest  cases  lasts 
about  15  msec.  (fig.  28),  decreasing  continuously 
from  its  peak  value  during  this  interval.  It  is  likely, 
although  this  has  not  as  yet  been  generally  estab- 
lished, that  the  synaptic  drive  of  the  testing  volley  is 
also  increased  by  antecedent  activity  of  the  nerve 
fibers.  This  enhancement  may  take  place  in  the  pre- 
synaptic fibers  themselves.  For  example,  invasion  of 
the  terminal  branches  by  the  conductile  activity  may 
be  partial  for  a  single  volley  and  larger  for  a  subse- 
quent. Also,  the  amount  of  transmitter  released  by 
the  second  activity  may  be  larger.  In  many  junc- 
tional systems,  the  prolonged  stimulation  of  the  pre- 
synaptic nerve  at  relatively  high  frequencies  for 
some  time  thereafter  increases  the  effects  produced 
by  a  subsequent  single  testing  stimulus  (76,  87,  124, 
135,  139).  This  phenomenon,   post-tetanic  (cf    118) 


or  postactivating  (59)  potentiation,  may  likewise 
depend  upon  the  mechanisms  just  described.  In- 
creased synaptic  drive  may  also  involve  the  post- 
synaptic membrane  as,  for  instance,  by  a  temporary 
change  in  the  excitability  of  the  membrane  to  the 
transmitter  agent.  These  residual  presynaptic  and 
postsynaptic  effects  may  alter  synaptic  drive  in  either 
direction  and  act  without  relation  to  the  residual 
p.s.p.  from  the  first  volley.  Thus,  the  homosynaptic 
facilitation  which  occurs  in  the  eel  electroplaque 
(fig.  29)  lasts  for  about  i  sec,  whereas  the  p.s.p. 
lasts  only  2  to  3  msec.  (4,  5,  6). 

e)  HETEROSYN.'SiPTic  FACILITATION.  Hetcrosynaptic 
testing  eliminates  the  complications  introduced  in 
homosynaptic  facilitation  except  the  refractoriness 
of  the  discharged  postjunctional  cells.  The  facilita- 
tion now  may  start  at  very  brief  intervals  between 
the  stimuli.  Strictly  speaking,  however,  hetcro- 
synaptic testing  involves  spatial  factors  for  the 
terminations  of  one  pathway  may  activate  different 
synaptic  sites  than  do  those  of  the  other.  The  facilita- 
tion is  therefore  likely  to  take  place  by  electrotonic 
additions  of  the  depolarization  produced  by  the  test- 
ing stimulus  onto  the  residual  level  of  general  de- 


1 86 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


ZOO 


MSEC 

FIG.  29.  The  time  course  of  homosynaptic  facilitation  in  a  group  of  eel  electroplaques.  The  testing 
stimulus  alone  evoked  responses  shown  by  the  horizontal  lines  at  the  end  of  the  graph  in  B.  At 
various  intervals  after  a  conditioning  stiinulus,  the  response  to  the  test  stimulus  became  larger  than 
this  control  value  and  gradually  returned  toward  it.  •  :  Facilitation  without  treatment  with  drug. 
A :  1 2  min.  after  adding  50  >jg  per  ml  physostigmine  the  cells  became  somewhat  more  excitable, 
the  whole  curve  of  facilitation  being  lifted  on  the  baseline  of  the  larger  response  to  the  testing  stim- 
ulus in  isolation.  This  effect  presumably  developed  because  of  the  anticholinesterase  action  of  phy- 
sostigmine; it  is  also  produced  by  prostigmine.  The  two  substances,  however,  have  opposite  synaptic 
action,  physostigmine  being  an  inactivator  of  synaptic  electrogenesis  and  prostigmine  an  excitant, 
n  ■  64  min.  later,  the  physostigmine  had  depressed  synaptic  excitability  and  the  whole  curve  had 
fallen.  Expressed  in  percentile  values  of  the  response  to  the  testing  stimulus  alone  in  each  condition, 
the  three  curves  had  essentially  the  same  magnitudes  and  time  courses  (.4).  [From  .\ltamirano 
et  al.  (6).] 


polarization  remaining  from  the  prior  stimulus.  This 
is,  indeed,  the  condition  found  experimentally  (fig. 
30^)  in  the  electroplaques  from  the  Sachs  organ  of 
the  eel.  Both  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  being  short, 
facilitation  occurs  only  during  the  first  2  msec.  In 
cells  of  the  main  organ,  however,  heterosynaptic 
facilitation  lasts  some  50  to  75  msec.  (fig.  3oi?)  and 
in  this  case  the  effect  must  be  due  to  alteration  of  the 
excitability  of  the  synaptic  membrane  since  pre- 
synaptic interactions  are  ruled  out.  The  different 
behavior  of  the  electroplaques  in  the  two  organs  is 
probably  ascribable  to  difTerent  spatial  relations  of 
their  synapses.  If  those  in  the  electroplaques  of  the 
main  organ  are  closely  spaced,  diffusion  of  transmit- 
ter from  the  sites  activated  b\-  the  conditioning 
volley  might  affect  the  excitability  of  the  synaptic 
loci  innervated  by  the  second  neural  pathway  (95). 
The  data  presented  above  derive  from  a  particu- 
larly favorable  structural  configuration,  a  large 
postsynaptic  cell  with  an  extensive  responsive  mem- 


brane (about  15  mm-  in  area)  diffusely  innervated 
by  several  easily  isolated  nerve  trunks.  The  experi- 
mental conditions  that  obtain  in  nerve  cells  do  not 
usually  perinit  as  clear  a  delineation  between  dif- 
ferent spatial  interactions.  However,  in  the  case  of 
cells  with  long  dendrites,  as  in  the  cortex,  it  may  be 
expected  that  interaction  between  different  axo- 
somatic  synapses  will  be  greater  than  that  between 
these  and  the  axodendritic. 

f)  sp.'Kti.'^l  sum.m.ation  of  converging  p.^th\v.\ys. 
Another  variety  of  spatial  summation  is  more  fre- 
quently noted  in  the  central  nervous  system.  This  is 
the  case  in  which  two  widely  separated  neuronal 
complexes  eventually  converge  upon  one  or  more 
common  paths.  In  that  final  common  path  the  situa- 
tion then  reduces  to  a  variant  of  the  case  discussed 
above.  These  convergent  types  of  interaction  are 
further  complicated  in  the  central  nervous  system 
by    the    involvement    of   inhibitory    p.s.p.'s.    Spatial 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION  1 87 


FIG.  30.  Heterosynaptic  facilitatory  actions  in  eel  electroplaques  from  different  electric  organs. 
Left:  Absence  of  heterosynaptic  facilitation  in  cells  from  the  Sachs  organ.  .Activity  of  one  nerve 
evoked  the  response  seen  in  A.  This  response  -was  preceded  by  that  evoked  through  another  nerve  in 
B  to  G.  Only  when  the  two  stimuli  (shock  artifacts  on  the  left  of  the  records)  were  less  than  i  msec, 
apart  (_F,  G)  was  there  a  significant  amount  of  facilitation  caused  by  electrotonic  summation  of  the 
brief  p.s.p.  [From  .-Mtamirano  et  al.  (5).]  Right:  Heterosynaptic  facilitation  in  cells  of  the  main  organ. 
A:  The  response  to  stimulating  one  nerve  trunk.  B:  This  nerve  trunk  is  used  to  deliver  the  con- 
ditioning stimulus  (artifact  at  the  left,  upward);  the  testing  stimulus  is  applied  at  various  intervals 
later  to  another  nerve  trunk  (artifact  down,  superimposed  traces).  Marked  facilitation  reached  a 
peak  at  10  to  15  msec,  and  persisted  through  the  end  of  the  record  at  25  msec.  C:  Nerve  2  was  cut, 
and  a  third  nerve  trunk  was  used  for  the  testing  stimuli.  No  facilitation  occurred.  [From  Albe-Fes- 
sard  &  Chagas  (i).] 


inhibition  or  facilitation  may  develop  particularly 
in  the  more  complex  varieties  of  synaptic  organiza- 
tion. The  precise  effects  would  depend  on  the  specific 
pathways  and  electrical  responses  involved  and  can- 
not be  discussed  in  this  chapter  (cf.  99-101,  161). 


186).  Thus,  they  can  provide  sites  at  which  synaptic 
potentials  of  both  signs  may  be  generated  and  this 
electrical  summation  propagated  electrotonically  to 
act  upon  an  electrically  excitable  membrane  distal 
to  the  cell  body. 


Integrative  Utility  of  Electrical  Inexcitability 

The  foregoing  group  of  integrative  activities  de- 
pends essentially  upon  graded,  algebraically  sum- 
mative  potentials  of  opposite  signs  which  are  made 
available  in  synaptic  transmission  by  electrical  in- 
excitability. In  some  neurons,  relatively  large  scale 
areas  of  membrane  are  not  electrically  excitable  and 
this  would  appear  to  aid  integrative  functions.  The 
superficial  cortical  dendritic  surfaces,  richly  supplied 
with  synaptic  inflows,  are  an  example  of  this.  The 
synaptic  activity  that  goes  on  at  these  dendrites  re- 
sult in  algebraically  summated  potentials.  Since 
these  dendrites  are  not  electrically  excitable,  the  po- 
tentials must  be  transmitted  electrotonically  to  the 
electrically  excitable  membrane  of  the  pyramidal 
neurons.  In  each  of  these  the  potential  can  serve  to 
modulate  responsiveness  to  other,  more  potent 
synaptic  inflows.  The  soma  of  lobster  cardiac  gan- 
glion cells  also  are  not  electrically  excitable  (33,  109, 


Synaptic  Determinants  of  Different  Types  of  Reflexes 

In  the  general  context  of  principles,  the  precise 
structural  and  functional  complexity  of  a  reflex 
pathway  is  of  little  moment.  Therefore,  the  specific 
properties  of  monosynaptic  or  multisynaptic  reflexes 
need  not  be  dwelt  upon  since  they  are  finally  refer- 
able to  the  intensity  of  synaptic  drives  upon  the  final 
common  path.  The  analysis  of  synaptic  mechanisms 
in  many  varieties  of  reflex  response  can  likewise  be 
simplified  by  merging  all  interneuronal  activities  with 
that  of  the  final  common  path,  essentially  involving 
a  reduction  to  the  monosynaptic  case. 

Synaptic  organizations  involving  very  strong 
synaptic  drive  for  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  will  manifest 
themselves  by  large  synchronized  efferent  electrical 
activity  or  a  twitch-like  contraction  in  response  to  a 
single  afferent  volley.  The  amplitude  of  the  response 
will  depend  upon  the  proportion  of  neurons  that  lie 
in  the  discharged  zone.  The  lower  the  proportion  of 


1 88 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


cells  with  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  above  the  critical 
level,  the  greater  will  be  the  degree  of  facilitation  on 
repetitive  stimulation.  If  the  majority  of  the  cells  in 
the  excited  zone  develop  p.s.p.'s  only  slightly  below 
the  level  of  critical  depolarization,  a  few  repetitive 
stimuli  will  rapidly  evoke  a  maximal  response.  This 
gives  rise  to  the  general  class  of  cPemblee  reflexes 
(cf.  44).  Augmentation  of  p.s.p.'s  (synaptic  facilita- 
tion) discussed  earlier  (p.  168)  also  will  favor  pro- 
duction o{  d' em blee  refiexes. 

When  the  p.s.p.'s  of  the  excited  zone  are  small, 
the  responses  may  recruit  very  gradually  with  repeti- 
tive stimuli.  Particularly  when  the  synaptic  organi- 
zation is  complex  and  the  synaptic  drives  are  weak 
will  the  latency  of  the  response  be  long.  Under  these 
conditions  it  may  also  be  expected  that  moer  fre- 
quent stimuli  will  shorten  the  latency  markedly  and 
increase  the  rate  of  growth  of  the  response  and  per- 
haps its  maximum  value.  In  other  words,  the  more 
weakly  effective  synaptic  drives,  including  multi- 
synaptic  pathways,  will  show  a  greater  frequency- 
dependence.  Since  the  production  of  hyperpolarizing 
p.s.p.'s  also  involves  excitation  of  synapses,  the  de- 
velopment of  inhibitory  activity  will  depend  similarly 
upon  the  stimulus  parameters. 

Another  effect  in  which  the  complexits  of  the 
synaptic  organization  plays  a  role  is  that  of  after- 
discharge.  The  involvement  of  multisynaptic  path- 
ways carries  the  likelihood  that  additional  side 
paths  will  also  be  brought  into  activity  and  thus 
give  rise  to  a  circulating  activity  (78)  or  a  series  of 
delayed  reverberations  which  may  cause  discharges 
of  the  final  common  path  long  after  the  initial 
stimulus  is  ended.  This  reverberation  may  take  place 
by  one-to-one  excitation,  but  it  is  likely  that  another 
phenomenon  plays  an  even  greater  role.  This  is  the 
summation  and  persistence  of  p.s.p.'s  associated  with 
accumulation  of  a  persistent  transmitter  agent.  As 
individual  Renshaw  cells  are  capable  of  persistent 
repetitive  discharge  by  a  single  stimulus  (61),  so 
some  of  the  interneurons  mediating  excitatory 
p.s.p.'s  can  also  remain  active  for  a  long  time.  The 
interplay  of  excitatory  and  inhibitory  synaptic 
activity  may  produce  complex  patterns  of  waxing  and 
waning  after  discharge.  In  individual  cells  this  pat- 
terning would  be  reflected  by  a  greater  or  lower 
frequency  of  discharge.  Complex  interactions  of 
excitatory  and  inhibitory  types  occur  even  in  the 
relatively  simple  nuclear  structures  like  that  of 
Clarke's  column  in  the  spinal  cord  (104,  115).  The 
involvement  of  a  widespread  network  of  neuron 
complexes  in  after-discharge  is  indicated  also  by  the 


fact  that  increasing  the  strength  of  the  initiating 
stimulus  may  lead  to  no  increase  in  the  maximal 
amplitude  of  the  reflex  respon.se  but  only  in  the  dura- 
tion of  its  after-discharge  (182). 

Role  of  Inlnhitioii  in  Central  Nervous  System 

The  interrelations  of  depolarizing  and  hyper- 
polarizing p.s.p.'s  in  the.se  various  manifestations, 
in.sofar  as  they  are  dependent  upon  the  specific 
organization  of  synapses,  are  beyond  the  scope  of 
this  chapter,  but  some  general  discussion  is  ap- 
propriate (cf.  1 01,  161).  As  was  described  earlier, 
hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  need  attain  only  relatively 
small  amplitudes  to  produce  inhibition.  The  effect, 
a  sudden  cutting  off  of  conductile  activity,  may 
block  the  synaptic  transfer  to  many  systems  which 
would  normally  participate  in  an  activity.  The  re- 
sults of  a  given  excitatory  and  inhibitory  interaction 
will  differ  depending  upon  the  site  at  which  an  index 
of  the  effect  is  obtained.  In  a  specific  example  let  us 
assume  that  a  single  cell  is  acted  upon  by  the  synaptic 
interplay.  Whether  or  not  it  is  excited  to  produce  a 
spike  will  have  important  consequences  for  the  ac- 
tivity of  other  downstream  neurons  for  which  the 
cell  chosen  as  an  example  serves  as  a  valve.  How- 
ever, when  recording  from  the  interior  of  the  cell, 
depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing  activities  may  be 
oi)served  even  in  the  absence  of  a  spike.  Thus,  dif- 
ferent criteria  apply  to  activity  in  different  parts  of 
a  complex  pathway.  The  relations  between  activity 
in  one  part  and  another  may  even  be  dimmed  or 
may  disappear. 

The  activity  set  into  motion  by  a  synaptically 
complex  pathway  thus  may  be  undetected  in  the 
overt  response.  For  example,  a  single  stimulus  to  the 
head  of  the  caudate  nucleus  in  the  cat  giv-es  rise  to  a 
relatively  simple,  brief  electrical  response  in  a  re- 
stricted cortical  region.  Analyses  with  paired  or 
repetitive  stimuli  disclose  (167)  that  many  excitatory 
and  inhibitory  influences  are  activated,  some  for 
long  periods  of  time.  It  is  worth  noting  that  ana- 
tomical data  can  rarely  give  information  as  to  the 
presence  of  such  intricate  synaptic  linkages  and,  of 
course,  cannot  distinguish  those  that  are  excitatory 
from  the  inhibitory. 

It  is  most  likely  that  in  its  normal  functioning  the 
central  nervous  system  utilizes  inhibitory  activity  as 
a  means  for  braking  excitatory  activity  which  might 
otherwise  be  unduly  prolonged  or  inclined  to  rever- 
beration. In  that  sense,  therefore,  inhiijitory  synaptic 
electrogenesis   would    aid    the    precision    of  ner\ous 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


189 


activity.  The  remo\al  of  inhibitory  electrogenesis 
either  by  drugs  which  specifically  blockade  hyper- 
polarizing  synapses,  or  by  pathological  conditions, 
would  then  remove  these  brakes  upon  excitatory 
activity  and  abnormal  function  would  result.  This 
would  be  apparently  caused  by 'excitation'  although 
its  fundamental  mechanism  would  in  reality  be  the 
block  of  another,  opposed  type  of  synaptic  activity, 
the  inhibitory  electrogenesis.  The  pharmacological 
classification  of  strychnine  as  a  'stimulant  of  the 
central  nervous  system',  already  discussed,  illustrates 
the  basic  difference  between  a  descriptive,  phe- 
nomenological  classification  and  that  ba.sed  on 
analysis  of  its  mode  of  action. 

Phynulogical  Effects  of  Different  Proportions  oj  De- 
polarizing and  Hyperpolarizing  Postsynaptic  Potentials 

Apparently  different  physiological  and  pharma- 
cological properties  may  result  from  different  pro- 
portions of  the  two  kinds  of  synaptic  activity.  Thus, 
the  electrical  activity  of  the  cat  cerebral  cortex 
differs  profoundly  from  that  of  the  cerebellar,  but 
these  differences  may  be  accounted  for  by  the  rela- 
tively small  degree  of  inhibitory  electrogenesis  ol  the 
cerebellar  cortex  (161-166).  Pharmacological  dif- 
ferences, such  as  the  insensitivity  of  the  cerebellar 
cortex  to  local  applications  of  strychnine,  are  equally 
ascribable  to  this  quantitative  factor. 

However,  the  response  of  the  cerebellar  electro- 
cortical  activity  to  different  drugs  depends  upon 
the  mode  of  exciting  that  activity  (Purpura,  Girado  & 
Grundfest,  in  preparation;  cf.  also  99-101,  163). 
Different  cerebellopetal  afferents  may  evoke  po- 
tentials of  different  forms  at  a  single  cortical  site. 
These  potentials  are  composed  of  different  propor- 
tions of  excitatory  and  inhibitory  synaptic  activities 
as  demonstrated  by  their  different  reactions  to  the 
various  specifically  acting  drugs. 

Synaptic  Activity  and  Electrical  Concomitants 

The  matters  discussed  under  this  heading  relate 
physiological  activity  in  the  central  nervous  system 
to  the  methodology  of  its  study  by  electrophysiologi- 
cal means.  They  are  also  considered  by  Frank  in 
Chapter  X  of  this  work. 

a)  interpret.ations  of  changes  in  .'\mplitudes  of 
POSTSYNAPTIC  POTENTIALS.  Since  p.s.p.'s  are  not 
subject  to  refractoriness  but  are  capable  of  summa- 
tion and  of  being  sustained,  decrease  in  amplitudes  of 
p.s  p.'s  cannot  be  ascribed  to  their  refractoriness  or 


'occlusion'.  A  depolarizing  p.s.p.  therefore  can 
diminish  only  by  virtue  of  the  following  factors. 

/)  The  conductile  process  of  the  preceding  unit 
is  blocked  by  refractoriness.  This  is  probably  a  minor 
element  since  profound  alterations  in  synaptic  re- 
sponses occur  at  frequencies  of  repetitive  activity  so 
low  that  refractoriness  of  electrically  excitable  re- 
sponses does  not  occur. 

2}  The  transmitter  of  the  presynaptic  terminals 
may  become  exhausted  or  the  receptor  of  the  post- 
synaptic membrane  may  become  altered.  The  latter 
factor  has  been  discussed  in  connection  with  de- 
.sensitization  (p.  157). 

^)  Stimulation  at  high  frequencies  may,  however, 
produce  fused  sustained  p.s.p.'s  that  show  little  or 
no  fluctuation  from  the  steady  level  (fig.  gfi,  C). 
This  effect  probably  develops  when  the  synaptic 
membrane  is  maximally  excited  by  the  frequently 
released  packets  of  transmitter  agent.  The  steady 
depolarization  (or  hyperpolarization)  can  be  re- 
corded in  the  cerebral  cortex  (cf.  176,  fig.  10). 

4)  Simultaneous  and  countervailing  development 
of  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  may  mask  the  depolariz- 
ing. There  is  now  considerable  experimental  evidence 
that  this  factor  is  most  important  in  the  complex 
synaptic  organization  of  the  central  nervous  system 
(165-167).  Indeed,  the  overt  electrogenesis  ob- 
servable in  the  cerebral  cortex  after  a  single  stimulus 
may  be  only  a  small  part  of  the  total  electrogenic 
activity.  The  major  part  is  not  recorded  because 
depolarizing  and  hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  are  simul- 
taneously produced  and  tend  to  cancel  each  other. 

b)  interpretation  OF  electrotonic  effects  of 
STANDING  postsynaptic  POTENTIALS.  It  has  been 
frequently  assumed  that  the  surface  negativity  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  caused  by  dendritic  p.s.p.'s  produces 
anodal  polarization  of  their  cell  bodies  (cf.  i  76,  pp. 
56  and  57).  This  conclusion  is  drawn  from  analogy  with 
the  effects  of  externally  applied  currents,  a  cathode 
on  the  surface  depressing  and  a  surface  anode  aug- 
menting excitability  of  the  cell  bodies.  This  analogy 
is  not  valid  in  the  physiological  case.  Externally 
recorded  negativity  means  that  the  interior  of  the 
generating  site  is  depolarized  (i.e.  more  positive  than 
the  resting  potential).  Surface  negative  p.s.p.'s  of 
apical  dendrites  therefore  must  always  act  as  an  ex- 
citatory (cathodal)  stimulus  for  the  electrically  ex- 
citable membrane  of  their  cells  (cf.  97,  loi). 

c)  synaptic  transducer  action  AND  electro- 
genesis. The  recorded  electrical  activity  might  even 


I  go 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


STIMULUS 
Subthreshold  Thretshold 


FIG.  31.  Ephaptic  excitation  of  squid  giant  axon.  Two  nerves 
are  arranged  as  siiown  in  diagram.  I.  Contact  between  nerves 
in  sea  water.  A  weak  stimulus  (/(/O  evokes  a  local  response  of 
the  pre-ephaptic  fiber  (seen  in  trace  .4).  This  is  not  propagated 
to  the  ephapse  and  has  no  effect  on  the  latter  (trace  B).  When 
a  pre-ephaptic  spike  was  evoked  by  a  stronger  stimulus  (.4'), 
the  post-ephaptic  nerve  generated  a  locaJ  response  (B').  .^head 
of  it  is  seen  the  electrotonic  pick-up  of  the  pre-ephaptic  spike. 
II.  Excitability  of  the  axons  was  increased  by  removing  calcium 
ions  from  the  medium.  The  weak  stimulus  still  could  not  evoke 
activity  in  the  post-ephaptic  fiber  (B),  since  conductile  activity 
was  lacking  in  the  pre-ephaptic  unit  QA).  When  a  stronger 
stimulus  evoked  a  spike  (.4')  the  postephaptic  fiber  also  pro- 
duced a  spike  (B')-  This  arose  on  a  step  which  is  the  local 
response  of  the  postephaptic  fiber  (seen  in  isolation  on  the 
lowest  trace).  [From  Arvanitaki  (10).] 


be  absent  if  ionic  processes  leading  to  hyperpolariz- 
ing  and  depolarizing  p.s.p.'s  were  equally  balanced. 
Despite  this,  however,  the  transducer  actions  ini- 
tiated by  the  excitants  of  depolarizing  and  hyper- 
polarizing  synaptic  membrane  would  still  take  place, 
and  the  ionic  transports  of  the  transducer  action 
would  still  occur.  Thus,  ionic,  metabolic  and  other 
biochemical  effects  might  be  produced  in  the  ap- 
parent absence  of  electrical  activity  (96,  97). 


EPH.APTIC    EXCITATION 

Electrical  Modes  of  Transmission 

In    the   course   of  efforts    to   validate   the    theory 
of  electrical  transmission  many  attempts  were  made 


to  confirm  Kiihne's  dictum  that  "a  nerve  only  throws 
a  mu.scle  into  contraction  by  means  of  its  currents 
of  action."  In  1882,  Hering  (i  10)  found  that  a  nerve 
\-olley  initiated  in  one  distal  branch  of  the  frog 
sciatic  nerve  and  coursing  centrally  in  the  whole 
nerve  trunk  could  set  up  activity  in  another  branch 
when  the  impulses  arrived  at  the  centrally  transected 
stump  of  the  nerve.  The  current  flow  generated  by 
the  active  fibers  must  have  stimulated  the  previously 
inactive  fibers.  The  effect  has  been  confirmed  many 
times  (cf.  149)  but  nowhere  more  clearly  than  in  a 
preparation  involving  two  squid  giant  axons  (lo). 
It  must  be  emphasized  that  specially  favorable  ex- 
perimental conditions  are  required  to  produce  this 
■  model'  of  transmission  which  is  termed  an  '  ephap.se' 
(false  synapse).  In  the  squid  giant  fiber  (fig.  31)  the 
electrical  excitability  of  the  ephaptic  region  is 
heightened  by  depriving  the  medium  of  calcium. 
The  extrinsic  current  of  the  spike  in  the  pre-ephaptic 
terminal  is  then  capable  of  acting  as  a  sufficiently 
strong  electrical  stimulus  to  evoke  a  postephaptic 
spike.  As  a  weaker  stimulus,  it  can  elicit  a  graded 
local  response.  In  more  complex  geometrical  con- 
ditions between  active  and  inactive  cells,  the  direc- 
tions of  the  extrinsic  or  field  currents  may  produce 
hyperpolarizations  as  well  as  depolarizations  (figs. 
I,  32).  The  activity  travelling  in  one  fiber  generates 
extrinsic  current  fields  in  contiguous  parallel  fibers 
which  have  a  triphasic  sequence  (126)  that  suc- 
cessively produces  hyperpolarization  and  depressed 
excitability,  then  depolarization  and  heightened 
excitability,  followed  again  by  hyperpolarization 
and  depression  (fig.  32). 

A  weakness  of  ephaptic  transmission  as  a  model 
of  synaptic  activity  lies  in  the  fact  that  basically  it 
does  not  offer  a  mechanism  for  polarized  transmis- 
sion. Thus,  in  figure  31  the  ephaptic  excitation  might 
very  well  have  taken  the  opposite  direction,  from 
nerve  B  to  nerve  A.  Special  geometric  properties 
were  invoked  by  du  Bois-Reymond  and  by  Eccles 
(figs.  I,  32),  and  tlie  latter  also  introduced  the  special 
electrophysiological  rectifying  effects  of  anodal  and 
cathodal  currents  (fig.  32).  These  conditions  might 
account  for  polarized  transmission  with  an  electrical 
mechanism;  and,  as  will  be  described  below,  a  high 
degree  of  rectification  recently  discovered  in  one  kind 
of  junction  (83)  does  polarize  conduction.  However, 
the  crucial  distinction  is  whether  or  not  current 
flow  in  a  presynaptic  terminal,  or  current  flow  im- 
posed through  the  synaptic  junction,  can  excite  the 
activity  of  the  latter.  The  an.swer,  illustrated  in  this 
chapter  with  a  number  of  examples  (e.g.  figs.  6,  19), 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


191 


120- 


110-   S 


100 


90- 


80- 


Action  potential  in 
fibre  I 


Excitability  chonge  in 
fibre  IE 


>A,X»-TC 


Ct1"<AAt 


i':i.|:>iaJ-t 


msec.  * —  * — 

FIG.  32.  Excitability  changes  caused  by  field  currents.  I'pper  lejt:  A  spike  was  produced  by  a  stim- 
ulus to  one  of  a  pair  of  crab  nerve  fibers  as  in  diagram  upper  right.  The  electrical  excitability  of  the 
second  fiber  is  shown  (lower  left)  in  relation  to  the  time  at  which  the  spike  passed  the  testing  region. 
In  the  interval  before  the  spike  had  reached  that  site,  the  excitability  of  the  fiber  was  depressed. 
During  the  time  that  activity  resided  at  the  tested  level,  the  excitability  was  augmented.  This  was 
followed  by  a  second  depressed  phase  as  the  activity  propagated  out  of  the  tested  site.  [From  Katz  & 
Schmitt  (126).]  Right:  Diagrams  of  the  anodal,  cathodal  and  anodal  polarizing  sequence  generated 
in  the  inactive  fiber  by  the  spike  in  an  adjoining  fiber  ilop')  and  of  different  field  current  conditions 
produced  by  different  geometrical  arrangements  (bottom).   [From  Eccles  (57).] 


seems  to  be  clear.  The  current  flowing  across  an 
active  presynaptic  terminal  and  across  the  post- 
synaptic membrane  appears  to  be  far  too  small  to 
excite  the  postsynaptic  cell.  Furthermore,  the  proc- 
esses associated  with  synaptic  activity  cannot  be 
initiated  by  very  strong  applied  currents. 

Role  of  Field  Currents  in  Central  Nervous  System 

The  activity  of  masses  of  cells  or  fibers  in  the 
central  nervous  system  is  particularly  conducive  to 
development  of  field  currents  within  the  volume  of 
this  structure  (15).  This  fact  suggested  (88,  90)  that 
field  effects  might  play  a  role  in  determining  the 
peculiarities  of  central  nervous  properties.  The 
hypothesis  appeared  to  have  been  confirmed  by  anti- 
dromic stimulations  of  motoneurons  which  altered 
the  responses  of  contiguous  motoneurons  to  a  testing 
afferent  volley  (170).  That  conclusion,  however,  is 
invalidated  by  the  subsequent  finding  (171;  cf.  60) 
that  the  antidromic  stimuli  evoked  synaptic  activity 
within  the  spinal  cord  by  means  of  the  recurrent 
collaterals  of  the  motoneurons. 

Although  field  currents  undoubtedly  play  some 
role  (cf.   106),  their  wide  significance  must  now  be 


questioned  in  the  light  of  the  evidence  that  synaptic 
transmission  is  not  effected  by  electrical  stimuli. 
Changes  in  membrane  potential  produced  in  one 
cell  by  activity  of  contiguous  elements  appear  to  be 
small  (33,  59,  125),  although  effects  may  be  revealed 
by  tests  on  electrically  excitable  membrane  (106, 
126,  185).  However,  the  effects  exerted  electro- 
chemically  on  p.s.p.'s  (as  described  in  the  section  in 
this  chapter  on  the  nature  of  postsynaptic  potentials) 
are  probably  insignificant.  Thus,  electrical  ine.x- 
citability  renders  the  transmissional  process  insensi- 
tive to  fields  of  current  in  the  central  nervous  system 
(93).  Teleologically  considered,  this  is  probably  an 
advantage.  The  fields  must  shift  from  moment  to 
moment  as  the  loci  of  activity  shift  in  the  cellular 
mass  of  the  volume  conductor.  The  effects  of  these 
fields  must  therefore  be  highly  unspecific,  now  pro- 
ducing increase,  now  depression  of  electrical  ex- 
citability, actions  that  probably  would  disturb  the 
precision  of  organized  orderly  synaptic  transfer. 
Thus,  electrical  inexcitability  of  synaptic  membrane 
removes  a  major  hazard,  that  irregular  effects  of 
electric  fields  might  disrupt  the  patterned  activity 
of  the  central  nervous  system. 


192 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Dorsal  Root  Reflex 

Acting  upon  electrically  excitable  components, 
however,  field  currents  might  still  affect  central 
nervous  functioning.  For  example,  small  depolariza- 
tion of  a  cell  by  field  current  might  facilitate  its 
discharge  by  an  otherwise  subliminal  depolarizing 
p.s.p.  Likewise,  presynaptic  terminals  close  to  an 
active  synaptic  focus  might  be  subjected  to  con- 
siderable potential  change  (51),  an  action  which 
may  account  for  the  dorsal  root  reflex  (188).  This 
prolonged  centrifugal  discharge  of  dorsal  root  fibers 
is  evoked  with  a  latency  of  some  milliseconds  after 
the  same  or  other  dorsal  root  fibers  cany  a  volley 
centripetally.  The  dorsal  root  reflex  is  enchanced 
by  low  temperatures  as  is  the  motor  root  reflex  (29, 
89).  The  latency,  temperature  effect  and  prolonged 
discharge  of  the  dorsal  root  reflex  indicate  that  it  is 
produced  by  a  synaptic  activity  in  the  spinal  cord, 
and  yet  there  is  no  histological  evidence  of  synaptic 
inflows  to  the  dorsal  root  collaterals.  In  the  absence 
of  the  latter,  ephaptic  excitation  may  be  invoked, 
but  will  involve  synaptic  pathways  also.  This  could 
result  from  the  field  effects  generated  in  the  dorsal 
root  terminals  by  the  activity  of  some  interneuronal 
pools.  The  activity  of  these  cells,  being  evoked  by 
synaptic  transfer,  would  account  for  the  apparent 
synaptic  properties  of  the  dorsal  root  reflex,  but  its 
final  development  would  be  by  ephaptic  excitation. 

Ephaptic    Transmission   in   Annelid  and 
Crustacean  Nerve  Cords 

In  many  species  of  these  invertebrates  there  occur 
junctions  (septa)  between  anatomically  distinci 
elements,  the  segments  of  the  septate  giant  axons. 
Across  the  septa  considerable  electrotonic  current 
flow  can  take  place  and  ephaptic  electrical  transmis- 
sion is  then  possible  (125;  Kao,  C.  Y.  &  H.  Grund- 
fest,  manuscript  in  preparation).  The  junctional 
membranes  of  these  functional  ephapses  must  there- 
fore be  fundamentally  different  from  those  of 
synapses,  across  which  only  insignificant  electrotonic 
current  flow  occurs.  However,  the  anatomical  data 
to  account  for  this  difference  are  still  unsatisfactory. 
The  transverse  sheaths  which  separate  abutting  seg- 
ments of  the  septate  giant  axons  appear  to  be  identi- 
cal with  the  sheaths  that  invest  the  axis  cylinders 
(cf.  1 25).  On  the  other  hand,  the  junction  between 
the  medial  giant  axon  and  the  motor  giant  fiber 
of  crayfish  seems  to  be  formed  by  processes  from  the 
postjunctional  motor  nerve  which  penetrate  the 
Schwann  sheaths  to  make  intimate  contact  with  the 


cell  membrane  of  the  prejunctional  fibers  Ci74)-  The 
junctions   between   two  motor  giant  axons  are  also 

similar. 

a)  unpol.\rized  EPH.'SiPTic  JUNCTIONS  Thcsc  havc 
been  studied  with  intracellular  recordings  in  the 
.septate  giant  axons  of  earthworm  (125)  and  cray- 
fish (Kao,  C.  Y.  &  H.  Grundfest,  manuscript  in 
preparation).  The  septa,  sometimes  called  'un- 
polarized  macros\napses'  (cf.  30,  125),  appear  to  be 
merely  the  boundaries  demarcating  the  multiple 
origins  of  the  .septate  giant  axons  from  a  number  of 
segments  of  the  animal.  Activity  in  one  segment  of 
the  axon  causes  electrotonic  potentials  in  the  neigh- 
boring segments  large  enough  to  excite  the  latter. 
Thus,  transmission  is  by  local  circuit  excitation,  es- 
sentially as  in  other  axons.  As  in  the  latter,  the 
ephaptic  transmission  of  the  septate  axons  is  un- 
polarized,  capable  of  propagating  an  impulse  in 
either  direction. 

b)  pol.-^rized  eph.-vptig  transmission.  One  system 
recently  described  (83),  the  junction  between  cord 
giant  fibers  and  efferent  motor  giant  axons  of  cray- 
fish, may  be  classified  in  this  category.  Current  flow- 
ing outward  from  the  depolarized  prefiber  can  enter 
the  junctional  membrane  of  the  postfiber,  causing 
large  depolarization  in  the  latter  (fig.  33.-1)  and  its 
ephaptic  excitation.  However,  when  the  postfiber  is 
depolarized  (fig.  335)  the  electrotonic  effects  in  the 
prefiber  are  small.  Likewise  when  the  prefiber  is 
hyperpolarized  current  flow  in  the  postfiber  is  hind- 
ered (fig.  33.4),  while  hyperpolarizing  the  postfiber 
causes  large  electrotonic  changes  in  the  prefiber 
(fig.  33^).  The  junctions  thus  exhibit  rectification, 
with  conductance  in  one  direction  (that  tending  to 
depolarize  the  postfiber)  about  20  times  greater  than 
in  the  opposite  direction.  Thus,  in  the  case  of  the 
motor  giant  fiber  ephapse,  the  low  electrical  resist- 
ance in  one  direction  and  high  resistance  in  the  other 
makes  for  polarized  ephaptic  transmis.sion. 

Since  the  junction  meets  the  criteria  of  anatomical 
discontinuity  and  transmissional  polarization,  it 
fits  the  definition  of  synapse  extant  since  Ramon  y 
Cajal  and  Sherrington.  However,  though  it  may  be 
called  an  'electrically  excitable  synapse'  (83),  it 
probably  differs  profoundly  from  the  electrically 
inexcitable  synapses  discussed  in  this  chapter.  The 
distinction  between  ephaptic  junctions  which  have 
low  electrical  resistance  and  synapses  which  have 
high  resistance  helps  to  make  the  classification  more 
precise.  Thus,  experiments  similar  to  those  shown  in 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


'93 


Vtt 


MV 


hypcrpolorizotion 


8 


5H 


'9^ 


^'post 


i^ 


hypcrpolarlzotion 


depolarizotion 


-5 


'pre 


•-lO 
mV 


FIG.  33.  Rectification  at  the  junction  be- 
tween a  cord  giant  fiber  and  a  motor  giant 
axon  in  crayfish  results  in  polarized  ephaptic 
transmission.  A:  Current  was  allowed  to  flow 
through  a  microelectrode  in  the  prejunctional 
cord  giant  axon.  The  changes  in  the  membrane 
of  the  same  fiber  were  recorded  with  another 
microelectrode  and  are  shown  on  the  abscissa. 
The  ordinate  indicates  the  membrane  voltage 
recorded  at  the  same  time  with  a  micro- 
electrode in  the  postjunctional  fiber.  When 
the  prefiber  was  depolarized,  a  steeply  rising 
depolarizing  change  also  took  place  in  the 
postfiber.  As  an  extrinsic  local  circuit  change 
was  produced  by  a  spike  in  the  prefiber,  it 
would  lead  to  an  electrically  excited  response 
of  the  postfiber.  When  the  prefiber  is  hyper- 
polarized  (left  side  of  .-1),  only  small  changes 
in  potential  develop  in  the  postfiber.  The  ratio 
of  current  flowing  in  the  two  directions  is  about 
20:1.  B:  In  this  experiment  current  was 
applied  to  the  postfiber,  the  abscissa  shows  the 
change  in  membrane  potential  of  this  fiber 
and  the  ordinate  the  change  in  membrane 
potential  of  the  prefiber.  When  the  postfiber  is 
hyperpolarized,  there  is  a  considerable  current 
How  into  it  from  the  prefiber,  causing  some 
clectrotonic  hypcrpolarization  of  the  latter. 
When  the  postfiber  is  depolarized,  little 
current  flows  into  the  prefiber  and  it  there- 
fore cannot  be  stimulated  by  a  spike  in  the 
postfiber.  Electrical  excitation  across  the 
junction  is  thus  transmitted  only  from  the 
pre-  to  the  postfiber.  [From  Furshpan  & 
Potter  (83).] 


figure  33  were  done  on  the  squid  giant  axon  synapse 
by  Tasaki.  He  "could  not  detect  any  recognizable 
spread  of  clectrotonic  effects  across  the  synapse  in 
either  direction"  (personal  communication).  It  is 
likely  that  pharmacological  data  and  various  other 
criteria  of  the  constellations  listed  in  table  i  will 
distinguish  the  two  types  of  transmission  systems 
further. 

Several  properties  of  the  polarized  ephaptic  junc- 
tion may  be  deduced  from  the  available  data  and 
from  general  considerations.  Rectification  is  ex- 
hibited by  the  membranes  of  many,  though  not  all, 
cells,  although  not  to  the  same  large  degree  (cf. 
Tasaki,  Chapter  III).  Where  found,  it  is  manifested 
by  a  higher  membrane  resistance  to  inward  current 
than  to  outward  flow.  In  the  present  case  two  mem- 
branes are  involved  and,  if  both  are  rectifiers,  then 
they  must  each  act  in  opposite  polarity  to  the  other. 
On  the  other  hand,  only  one  of  the  two  membranes 
need  show  rectification  and  this  situation  is  the  more 


probable.  It  also  seems  most  likely  that  this  property 
resides  in  the  surface  of  the  prefiber  for  in  that  case 
the  membrane  would  permit  outward  current  flow 
and  restrict  inward  as  in  other  cells.  The  membrane 
of  the  postfiber  would  then  need  have  no  rectifier 
properties  but  would  resemble  that  of  the  septa  in 
its    low    nondirectional    resistivity. 

As  may  be  seen  from  figure  34,  current  probably 
flows  outward  across  the  prejunctional  membrane 
during  the  ephaptic  transmissional  process  whereas 
in  the  rest  of  the  active  region  the  membrane  current 
is  inward.  Furthermore,  excitation  of  the  postfiber 
must  occur  at  membrane  sites  where  the  local  circuit 
current  flows  outward,  not  at  the  ephaptic  region 
where  it  flows  inward.  Therefore,  neither  junctional 
membrane  of  this  polarized  ephapse  takes  part  in 
the  active  responses  of  the  junction.  Like  the  mem- 
branes at  the  septa  they  therefore  need  not  be  ex- 
citable. 


194 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH'lSKJLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGV    I 


Intrinsic 


\ 


Local  Circuits 
-> 1        I ^ 


Transjunctional 


Ephaptic  Junction 

FIG.  34.  Diagiam  showing  the  current  flows  that  probably 
take  place  at  a  polarized  ephaptic  junction.  In  the  prejunc- 
tional fiber  membrane  current  flow  is  inward  in  the  region  of 
activity.  Longitudinal  current  flow  takes  place  behind  this 
region  as  part  of  the  intrinsic  local  circuit  within  this  fiber. 
Current  flows  outward  through  the  membrane  recovering  from 
previous  activity.  Outward  current  also  flows  in  the  prejunc- 
tional membrane  of  the  ephapse.  This  enters  the  postcphaptic 
cell  at  its  junctional  membrane  and  flows  out  through  adjacent 
regions  of  membrane,  exciting  the  latter.  Note  the  profound 
difference  between  the  current  flows  postulated  for  ephaptic 
transmission  in  this  diagram  and  the  hypothetical  situation  al 
synaptic  junctions  shown  in  fig.   i . 


Evolutiormry  Aspects  of  Ephaplii  Transmissum 

In  their  transverse  divisions  the  septate  axons  bear 
the  sign  of  their  segmental  origin.  The  processes  of  a 
number  of  neurons  at  a  nerve  cord  segment  fuse  to 
produce  a  short  length  of  giant  axon.  End-to-end 
apposition  of  the  segmental  fibers  then  forms  a  long 
axonal  pathway.  To  the  extent  that  the  septa  dis- 
appear or  that  their  resistance  is  low  the  segmented 
axons  approach  the  nonsegmented  giant  axons  in 
efliciency  as  through  conduction  pathways,  excited 
by  local  circuit  action. 

The  septate  axons,  however,  combine  with  through 
conduction,  another  feature  which  is  absent  in  the 
nonsegmented  giant  fibers  (Kao,  C.  Y.  &  H.  Grund- 
fest,  manuscript  in  preparation).  They  make  elaborate 
local  synaptic  connections,  both  efferent  and  afferent, 
with  other  fibers  of  the  nerve  cord.  Although  the 
anatomy  of  these  connections  is  not  as  yet  clear,  the 


synaptic  properties  of  the  septate  axons  probably 
derive  from  their  segmental  origin  of  the  fibers.  The 
septate  giant  axons  therefore  play  a  much  greater 
role  in  the  integrative  activity  of  the  nervous  system 
than  can  the  nonseptate  axon  which  lack  these  synap- 
tic connections  (103,  125). 

On  the  basis  of  the  interpretation  given  in  the  pre- 
vious paragraphs,  the  polarized,  electrically  excitable 
ephaptic  junction  may  be  derived  from  the  septal 
junctions  by  the  addition  of  rectifier  property  to 
one  of  the  junctional  membranes.  Two  other  features 
further  strengthen  the  resemblance  between  septate 
and  motor  giant  fibers.  The  two  motor  axons  of  a 
segment  make  unpolarized  connections  with  each 
other.  In  this  case,  too,  electron  microscopy  has  not 
as  yet  revealed  essential  details  (cf.  i  74).  Also,  like 
the  septate  axons,  the  motor  giant  fiber  combines 
'chemically  mediated  synapses'  with  an  ephaptic 
junction  (83).  The  former  presumably  are  electrically 
inexcitable. 

Thus,  it  appears  likely  that  motor  giant  fibers  of 
the  crayfish  bear  a  close  functional  similarity  to  the 
septate  axons  but  with  a  significant  modification  away 
from  the  latter.  It  remains  to  be  seen  whether  ephap- 
tic polarized  transmission  made  possible  by  rectifica- 
tion is  a  fairly  common  evolutionary  variant.  Another 
interesting  correlation,  whether  or  not  this  transmis- 
sion scheme  developed  only  in  those  animals  that 
have  septate  unpolarized  ephapses,  might  give  fur- 
ther  clues    to   their   evolutionary   origin. 

Qjiasiartificial  Synapses 

The  excitation  of  giant  nerve  fillers  in  annelid 
nerve  cords  by  activity  in  other  giant  axons  is  well 
documented  (31)  and  may  be  an  ephaptic  phe- 
nomenon In  Protida  the  sites  of  transfer  vary  from  one 
occasion  to  another  and  have  been  termed  quasi- 
artificial  synapses.  These  systems  have  not  yet  been 
studied  with  intracellular  recording.  The  latter  could 
help  to  determine  whether  the  transmission  is  ephap- 
tic or  whether  it  is  associated  with  complex  synaptic 
phenomena  such  as  have  been  found  in  earthworms 
(125)- 


REFERENCES 

1.  Albe-Fessard,  D.   and  C.   Chagas.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  46: 

823.  '954 

2.  Albert,  A.  Ergehn.  Physiol.  49:  425,  1957. 

3.  Altamirano,  M.  .and  C.  VV.  Coates.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp. 
Physiol.  49:  69,  1957. 


4.  Altamirano,   M.,   C.   W.   Coates  and   H.   Grundfest. 
J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38:  319,  1955. 

5.  Altamirano,  M.,  C.  W.  Coates,  H.  Grundfest  and  D. 
Nachmansohn.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  37:  91,  1953. 

6.  Altamirano,  M.,  C.  W.  Coates,  H.  Grundfest  and  D. 
Nachmansohn.  Biochim.  el  biophys.  acta  16:  449,  1955. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


•95 


7.  Araki,  T.  and  T.  Otani.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18:  47^,   1955.  42. 

8.  Ariens,  E.J.  Experientia  13;  161,  1957.  43 

9.  Ariens,    E.   J.    and  J.    M.    van    Ro.ssum.    Arch,    mlernnt. 
pharmacodyn.  110:275,  1957.  44. 

10.  Arvanitaki,  a.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  8g,  1942. 

11.  Arvanitaki,  A.  and  N.   Chalazonites.  Arch.  sc.  physiol. 

■o:  95.  1956.  45- 

12.  Awapara,  J.,  A.  J.  Landua,  R.  Fuerst  and  B.  Seale. 

J.  Biol.  Chem.  187:  35,  1950.  46. 

13.  Bain,   J.    A.    In:  UUrastruclure    and    Cellular    Chemistry    0]  47. 
Neural  Tissue,  edited  by  H.  Waelsch.  New  \'ork:  Hoeber- 
Harper,  1957,  p.  139.  48. 

14.  Barlow,  R.  B.  Introduction  to  Chemical  Pharmacology.  New  49. 
York:  Wiley,  1955. 

15.  Barron,  D.  H.  and  B.  H.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  92:  50. 
276,  1938.  51. 

16.  Bazemore,  A.,  K.  A.  C.  Elliott  and  E.  Florey.  .Nature,  52. 
London  178:  1052,  1956. 

17.  Bennett,  M.  V.  L.,  S.  M    Grain  and  H.  Grundfest.  53. 
J.  Gen.  Physiol.  113:  325,  1957. 

18.  Bernard,  C.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  1  :  90,   1849.  54, 

19.  Bernstein,  J.   Vntersuchungen  uber  den  Errengungsvorgang  im 
Nerven-  und  Muskelsysteme.  ]rie\AiAheT%,  1871.  55. 

20.  Bernstein,  J.  Arch.  Physiol.  Leipzig,  1882,  p.  329. 

21.  Bishop,  G.  H.  Physiol.  Rev.  36:  376,  1956.  56. 

22.  BoNVALLET,  M.,  P.  Dell  AND  G.  HiEBEL.  Electroencephalog.  57. 
&  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  6:  119,  1954.  58. 

23.  Boyd,  I.  A.  and  A.  R.  Martin,  J.  Physiol.  132:  74,  1956.  59. 

24.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 

■'7:43'.  1952-  60. 

25.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.,  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 

122:  429,  1953.  61. 

26.  Brodie,   B.   B.   In:    Third  Conjerence  on  Neuropharmacology, 

edited  by  H.  A.  .■\biamson.  New  York:  Macy,   1957,  p.  62. 

323- 

27.  Brookhart,  J.  M.  and  .-\.  Zanchetti.  Electroencephalog.  &  63. 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  8:  427,  1956. 

•28.  Brooks,  C.  McG,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  J.  L.  Malcolm.  J.  64. 

Neurophysiol.   11:417,  1948.  65. 

29.  Brooks,  C.  McC,  K.  Koizumi  and  J.  L.  Malcolm.  J. 
Neurophysiol.  18:  205,  1955.  66. 

30.  Bullock,  T.  H.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Quant.  Biol.  1 7 : 

267,  1952.  67. 

31.  Bullock,  T.  H.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  98:  37,  1953. 

32.  Bullock,  T.  H.  and  S.  Hagiwara.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  40:  68. 

565.  1957-  69- 

33.  Bullock,  T.  H.  and  C.  A.  Terzuolo.  j'.  Physiol.  138:  341,  70. 

1957-  7'- 

34.  Burke,  W.  and  B.  L.  Ginsborg.  J.  Physiol.  132:  586,  1956.  72. 

35.  Burke,  W.  and  B.  L.  Ginsborg.  J.  Physiol.  132:  599,  1956.  73. 

36.  Burn,  J.  H.  Functions  of  Autonomic  Transmitters.  Baltimore:  74. 
Williams  &  Wilkins,  1956.  75. 

37.  Cerf,  J.,  H.  Grundfest,  G.   Hoyle  and  F.   McCann. 

Biol.  Bull.  113:  337,  1957.  76. 

38.  Cerf,  J.,  H.   Grundfest,  G.   Hoyle  and  F.   McCann.  77. 
Biol.  Bull.  113:  338,  1957. 

39.  Chagas,  C.  and  D.  Albe-Fessard.  Acta  physiol.  latinoam.  78. 

4:  49.  1954-  79- 

40.  Clark,  A.  J.  The  Mode  0]  Action  of  Drugs  on  Cells.  London: 
Arnold,  1933.  80. 

41.  CouTEAtix,    R.    In:    International   Review   of  Cytology.    New  8l. 
York:  Acad.  Press,  1955,  vol.  IV. 


Grain,  S.  M.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  104:  285,  1956. 

Grain,  S.   M.,  M.  V.  L.  Bennett  and  H.  Grundfest. 

Biol.  Bull.  113:  342,  1957. 

Creed,  R.  S.,  D.  Denny-Brown,  J.  C.  Eccles,  E.  G.  T. 

LiDDELL   AND   C.    S.    SHERRINGTON.    Reflex  AcHvily  of  the 

spinal  Cord.  Oxford:  Clarendon  Press,  1932. 

Curtis,  H.  J.  In:  Medical  Physiology  (loth  ed.),  edited  by 

P.  Bard.  St.  Louis:  Mosby,  1956,  p.  944. 

Dale,  H.  H.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  28:  319,  1935. 

Dallemagne,  J.  AND  E.   Philippot.  Acta  anaesth.  belg.  3: 

■25,  1952. 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  125:  546,  1954. 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  .Nature,  London  175:  1035, 

'955- 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  128:  396,  1955. 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  132:  630,  1956. 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  In:  Progress  in  Biophysics. 

London:  Pergamon,   1956,  vol.  6,  p.   121. 

DEL  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  ser. 

B  146:  339,   1957. 

de  Robertis,  E.  D.  p.  and  H.  S.  Bennett.  J.  Biopkys.  & 

Biochem.  Cylol.  i  :  47,  1955. 

DU  Bois-Reymond,    E.    Gesammelte  Abhandlungen  zur   allge- 

meinen  Muskel-  und  Nervenphysik.  Leipzig:  Veit,  1877,  ^^'-  '^• 

Eccles,  J.  C.  J.  Physiol.  loi  :  465,  1943. 

Eccles,  J.  C.  Ann.  New  fork  Acad.  Sc.  47:  429,  1946. 

Eccles,  ].  C.  J.  Neurophysiol.  g:  87,  1946. 

Eccles,  J.  C.  The  Neurophysiological  Basis  of  Mind.  O.xford: 

Clarendon  Press,  1953. 

Eccles,  J.    C.    The  Physiology  of  Nerve  Cells.    Baltimore: 

Johns  Hopkins  Press,  1957. 

Eccles,  J.  C,  P.  Fatt  and  K.  Koketsu.  J.  Physiol.  126: 

524.  '954- 

Eccles,  J.   C,  B.   Katz  and  S.  W.   Kuffler.  J.  Neuro- 
physiol. 4:  362,  1 94 1. 

Eccles,  J.  C.  and  W.  J.  O'Connor.  J.  Physiol.  94:  9(P), 
■938. 

Eccles,  R.  M.  J.  Physiol.  130:  572,  1955. 
Elkes,  J.  In:  Third  Conference  on  Neuropharmacology,  edited 
by  H.  A.  Abramson.  New  York:  Macy,  1957,  p.  205. 
Eyzacuirre,  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

87.  ■9,'J5- 

EvzAGUiRRE,  C.  AND  S.  W.  KuFFLER.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

'2'.    1955- 

Fatt,  P.  Physiol.  Rev.  34:  674,  1954. 

Fatt,  P.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  27,  1957. 

Fatt,  P.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  20:  61,  1957. 

Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  115:  320,  1951. 

Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  30:  433,  1953. 

Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  121  :  374,  1953. 

Feldberg,  W.  and  a.  Fessard.  J.  Physiol.  loi :  200,  1942. 

Feldberg,  W.,  J.  L.  Malcolm  and  S.  L.  Sherwood.  J. 

Physiol.  132:  130,  1956. 

Feng,  T.  P.  Chinese  J.  Physiol.  16:  341,  1941. 

Fessard,  A.  and  B.  H.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  95 :  P9, 

'939- 

Forbes,  A.  Physiol.  Rev.  2:  361,  1922. 

Frank,  K.  and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes.  J.  Physiol.   134:  451, 

1956. 

Freygang,  W.  H.,  Jr.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  41 :  543,  1958. 

Fulton,  J.   F.  Physiology  of  the  Nervous  System   (3rd   ed.). 

New  York:  Oxford,  1949. 


196 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


82.  FuoRTES,  M.  G.  F.,  K.  Frank  and  M.  C.  Becker.  J.  Gen. 
Physiol.  40:  735,  1957. 

83.  FuRSHPAN,  E.  J.   AND  D.  D.   PoTTER.  \alure,  London    180: 

342.  '957- 

84.  Galvani,  L.  De  viribus  electricitalis  in  rnolu  musculari  Com- 
mentarius,  1791.  English  translation  by  R.  M.  Green. 
Cambridge:  Licht,  1953. 

85.  GooDM.AN,  L.  S.  AND  A.  GiLMAN.  The  Pharmacological  Basts 
of  Therapeutics  (2nd  ed.).  New  York:  Macmillan,  1955. 

86.  GoPFERT,  H.  AND  H.  ScHAEFER.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  239: 

597.  1938- 

87.  Granit,  R.  J.  Physiol.  131 ;  32,  1956. 

88.  Grundfest,  H.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  2:  213,  1940. 

89.  Grundfest,  H.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  307,  1941. 

90.  Grundfest,  H.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  9:  477,  1947. 

91.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Electrochemistry  in  Biology  and  Medicine, 
edited  by  T.  Shedlovsky.  New  York :  Wiley,  1955,  chapt.  8. 

92.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Fi/tk  Conference  on  the  Nerve  Impulse. 
New  York:  Macy,  1956,  p.  177. 

93.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Problems  oj  Modern  Physiology  of  the 
Nerve  and  Muscle  System.  Tiflis:  Publ.  House,  Acad,  of 
Sciences,  Georgian  S.S.R.,  1956,  p.  81. 

94.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Physiological  Triggers.  Washington: 
Am.  Physiol.  Soc,  1957,  p.  119. 

95.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Progress  in  Biophysics.  London:  Perga- 
mon,  1957,  vol.  7,  p.  I. 

96.  Grundfest,  H.  Ann.  New  Tork  Acad.  Sc.  66:  537,  1957. 

97.  Grundfest,  H.  Physiol.  Rev.  37:  337,  1957. 

98.  Grundfest,  H.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  316,  1957. 
gg.  Grundfest,  H.  Fed.  Proc.  17:  1006,  ig58. 

100.   Grundfest,   H.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  In 

press, 
loi.   Grundfest,  H.  In:  Symposium  on  Reticular  Formation  of  the 

Brain,  edited  by  L.   D.   Proctor,  R.  S.  Knighton,   H.   H. 

Jasper,  W.  C.  Noshay  and  R.  T.  Costello.  Boston:  Little, 

1958,  P-  473- 

102.  Grundfest,  H.  Arch.  ital.  biol.  g6:  135,  ig58. 

103.  Grundfest,  H.  In:  Evolution  of  Nervous  Control  from  Primi- 
tive Organisms  to  Man.  AAAS  Symposium,  Section  on 
Medical  Science,  Dec.  2g,  1956.  To  be  published. 

104.  Grundfest,  H.  and  B.  Campbell.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  5:  275, 

1942- 

105.  Grundfest,  H.,  C.  Y.  Kao  and  M.  .<\lt.\mirano.  J.  Gen. 
Physiol.  38:  245,  ig54. 

106.  Grundfest,  H.  and  J.  Magnes.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   I64:  502, 

I951- 

107.  Grundfest,  H.  and  D.  P.  Purpura.  .Nature,  London   178: 

416,  igsS. 

108.  Grundfest,  H.  and  D.  P,  Purpura.  In:  Symposium  on 
Curare  and  Curarizing  Substances.  Amsterdam:  Elseviers,  igsg. 

109.  Hagiwara,  S.  and  T.  H.  Bullock.  J.  Cell  &  Comp. 
Physiol.  50:25,  1957. 

110.  Hering,  E.  Sitzber.  Akad.  W'iss.  Wien  {Abt.  Ill)  88:  237, 
1882. 

111.  Himwtch,  H.  E.  and  F.  Rinaldi.  In:  Brain  Mechanisms 
and  Drug  Action,  edited  by  W.  S.  Fields.  Springfield : 
Thomas,  1957,  p.  15. 

112.  HoDOKIN,  A.  L.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  148:  i,  1958. 

113.  HoDGKiN,  .\.  L.  AND  A.  F.  HuxLEV.  J.  Physiol.  117;  500, 

'952- 

114.  HoDOKiN,  A.  L.  AND  W.  A.  H.  RusHTON.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc, 
London,  ser.  fi  133:  444,   1946. 


i'5- 
116. 


I  i«. 
119. 


122. 
123. 

124. 

'25- 

126. 
127. 
128. 

i2g. 
130. 

■31- 
132. 

'33- 
134- 

■35- 
.36. 

137- 
138. 

139- 
140. 

141. 

142. 

'43- 
144. 

145- 
146. 

'47- 
148. 

>49- 
150. 

i5'- 


HoLMQvisT,  B.,  A.   Lundberg    and  O.   Oscarsson.    Acta 

physiol.  scandinav.  38:  76,  1956. 

HoYLE,    G.    In:    Recent   Advances   in   Invertebrate   Physiology. 

Eugene:  Univ.  Oregon  Press,  1957. 

HovLE,    G.    The   .Nervous    Control    of   Muscular   Contraction. 

London:  Cambridge,  ig57. 

Hughes,  J.  R.  Physiol.  Rev.  38:  91,  1958. 

HuTTER,  O.  F.  AND  W.  R.  LoWENSTEiN.  J.  Physiol.   1 30 : 

559.  1955- 

HuTTER,  O.  F.  AND  W.  Tr.'\utwein.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

715.  '956- 

Huxley,  A.  F.  In :  Progress  m  Biophysics.  London :  Perga- 
mon,  1957,  vol.  7,  p.  255. 

Ingvar,  D.  H.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  33:  169,  1955. 
Jenden,  D.  J.,  K.  Kamijo  and  D.  B.  Taylor.  J.  Pharma- 
col. &  Exper.   Therap.   ill:  229,  1954. 
Job,  C.  and  A.  Lundberg.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  28:  14, 

1953- 

Kao,  C.  Y.  .\fiD  H.  Grundfest.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  20:  494, 

'957- 

Katz,  B.  and  O.  H.  Schmitt.  J.  Physiol.  100:  369,  1942. 

Katz,  B.  and  S.  Thesleff.  J.  Physiol.  138:  63,  1957. 

Killam,  E.  K.  and  K.  F.  Killam.  In:  Brain  .Mechanisms  and 

Drug  Action,  edited  by  W.  S.  Fields.  Springfield :  Thomas, 

1957,  p.  71. 

Krivoy,  W.  A.  AND  J.  H.  Wills,  f.  Pharmacol.  &  Exper. 

Therap.  116:  220,  1956. 

Kuffler,  S.  W.  AND  C.  Eyzaguirre.  J.  Gen  Physiol.  39: 

155.  '955- 

Kuffler,  S.  W.  .\nd  B.  Katz.   J.   Neurophysiol.    9:   337, 

1946. 

Kuffler,    S.    W.    and    E.    M.    Vaughan-Williams.   J. 

Physiol.  121:  319,  1953. 

KiJHNE,  W.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  44:  427,  1888. 

LaPoRTE,  Y.   AND  R.   LORENTE  DE   N6.  J.   Cell.   &   Comp. 

Physiol.  Suppl.  2,  35:  61,  1950. 

Larrabee,  M.  G.  and  D.  W.  Bronk.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  10: 

139.  >947- 

Lettvin,  J.  Y.,  W.  S.  McCuLLOCH  and  W'.  Pitts.  Am.  J. 

Physiol.  187:614,  1956. 

LiLEY,  A.  W.  J.  Physiol.  134:  427,  1956. 

Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  525,  1941. 

Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  33:  147,  1949. 

Lloyd,   D.   P.   C.    In:    Textbook  of  Physiology  (17th   ed.), 

edited  by  J.  F.  Fulton.  Philadelphia.  Saunders,  1955,  p.  i. 

LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  A  Study  of  Nerve  Physiolog).  New  York: 

Rockefeller  Inst,  for  Med.  Res.,  1947,  vols.  131  and  132. 

Lorente  de  No,  R,   In:   The  Spinal  Cord.  Boston:  Little 

■953.  P-  '3 

LuFT,  J.  H.  J.  Biophys.  £?  Biochem.  Cytol.  2:  229,  1956. 

Lundberg,  A.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  35:  i,  I955. 

Lundberg,  A.  .Nature,  London  177:  1080,  1956. 

Lundberg,  A.  Physiol.  Rev.  38:  21,  1958. 

Magoun,  H.  W.  The  fi'aking  Brain.  Springfield:  Thomas, 

■938. 

Marrazzi,  .\.  S.  In;  Brain  Mechanisms  and  Drug  Action, 
edited  by  W.  S.  Fields.  Springfield:  Thomas,  1957,  p.  45. 
Marrazzi,  A.  S.  and  R.  Lorente  de  No.  J.  Neurophysiol. 

T-  83.  1944- 

MiNZ,   B.    The  Role  of  Humoral  Agents  in  Nervous  Activity. 

Springfield:  Thomas,  1955. 

Mountcastle,  V.  B.,  p.  \V.  Davis  and  .\.  L.  Berman.  J. 

Neurophysiol.  20:  374,   1957. 


SYNAPTIC    AND    EPHAPTIC    TRANSMISSION 


197 


152.  Palav,  S.  L.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem.  Cytol.  2:  Suppl.   193,  173. 
1956.  174- 

153.  Palay,  S.  L.  and  G.  E.  Palade.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem. 

Cytol.  i:  69,  1955.  175. 

154.  Parker,  G.  H.  The  Elementary  Nervous  System.  Philadelphia: 
Lippincott,  1919- 

155.  Paton,  VV.  D.  M.  Aim.  New  York  Acad.  Sc.  54;  347,  1951.  176. 

156.  Paton,  W.  D.  M.  and  W.  L.  M.  Perry.  J.  Physiol.  119: 

43.  1953- 

157.  Perry,  W.  L.  M.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  18:  279,  1956.  177. 

158.  Phillips,  C.  G.  Quarl.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  41  :  58,  1955. 

159.  Phillips,  C.  G.  Qiiarl.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  41 :  70,  1955. 

160.  Purpura,  D.  P.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  186:  250,  1956.  178. 

161.  Purpura,  D.  P.  In:  .^ympjsium  on  Reticular  Formation  oj  the 

Brain,  edited  by  L.   D.   Procter,  R.  S.   Knighton,  H.  H.  179. 

Jasper,    W.    C.   Noshay,   R.    T.   Costello.   Boston:   Little, 

1958, P- 435-  '8o- 

162.  Purpura,  D.  P.,  M.  Girado  and  H.  Grundfest.  Science 

125:  1200,  1957.  181. 

163.  Purpura,  D.  P.,  M.  Girado  T.  G.Smith,  D.  A.  Callan 

AND  H.   Grundfest.  J.  Nemochem.  1958.  In  press.  182. 

164.  Purpura,  D.   P.,  M.   Girado,  T.  G.  Smith   and  J.   A. 
Gomez.  Pruc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  97:  348,  1958.  183. 

165.  Purpura,  D.   P.  and  H.  Grundfest.  J.  Neurophysiol.   19: 

573.  1956-  184. 

166.  Purpura,  D.  P.  and  H.  Grundfest.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  20: 

494.  '957-  185. 

167.  Purpura,  D.  P.,  E.  M.  Housepian  and  H.  Grundfest. 

Arch,  ilal  biol.  96:  145,  1958.  ,gg 

168.  Ramon  \'  C.'\jal,  S.  Histologic  du  Systeme  .Nerveux  de  I'homme 

et  des  verlebres.   Madrid:  Consejo  Superior  de  Investigaci-  „ 

ones  Cientificas  Institute  "Ramon  y  Cajal",  1952,  2  vols. 

169.  Ram6n   y  Cajal,   S.  Neuron    Theory   or    Reticular    Theory? 
Objective   Evidence   oj  the   Anatomical    Unity   oj  Nerve   Cells,  "' 
translated   by   M.    U.    Purkiss   and   C.   A.   Fox.   Madrid:          '9°' 
Consejo  .Superior  de  Investigationes  Cientificas  Institute          '9'- 
"Ramon  y  Clajal ",  1954.                                                                       '9^- 

170.  Renshaw,  B.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  167,  1941..  '93- 

171.  Renshaw,  B.  J.  Neurophysiol.  9:  191,  1946. 

172.  Roberts,  E.  and  C.  Baxter.  Proc.  gth  Ann.  Mtg.  Am.  Acad.  194. 
Neurol.,  1957. 


Roberts,  E.  and  S.  Frankel.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  187:  55,  1950. 
Robertson,  J.   D.  J.   Biophys.  &   Biochem.  Cytol.   2:  381, 

1956- 

Robertson,  J.  D.  In:  Ultrastruclure  and  Cellular  Chemistry  oj 
Neural  Tissue,  edited  by  H.  Waelsch.  New  York :  Hoeber- 
Harper,  1957. 

Roitbak,  .\.  I.  Bioelectric  Phenomena  oj  the  Cerebral  Cortex 
(in  Russian).  Tiflis:  Publ.  House,  Acad,  of  Sciences, 
Georgian  S..S.R.,  1955. 

Rosenblueth,    a.    The    Transmission   oj   Nerve   Impulses   at 
Neuroejjector  Junctions  and  Periphrial  Synapses.   Cambridge : 
Technology  Press  and  New  York:  Wiley,  1950. 
Rothballer,  a.  B.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol. 
8:603,  1956. 

Scharrer,  E.  .\nd  B.  Sch.arrer.  In:  Handbuch  der  mikro- 
skopischen  Analomie  des  Menschen.  Berlin:  Springer,  1954. 
Sechenov,  I.  In:  Selected  Works  .\'V  International  Physiologi- 
cal Congress,  Moscow.  State  Publ.  House,  1935. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.  In :  A  Textbook  oj  Physiology  (7th  ed.), 
edited  by  M.  Foster.  London;  Macmillan,  1897. 
Sherrington,   C.   S.    The  Integrative  Action  oj  the  Nervous 
System,  1906.  Reprinted,  London:  Cambridge,  1947. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  £97:  519, 

■925- 

Svaetichin,  G.  Acta  physiol.  scandiimv.  suppl.   134,  39:  17, 

1956. 

Terzuolo,  C.  a.  .and  T.  H.  Bullock.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sc. 

42:  687,  1956. 

Terzuolo,  C.  .\.  .and  T.  H.  Bullock.  Arch.  ital.  biol.  In 

press. 

Thesleff,  S.  Acta  physiol.  scandmav.  34:  218,  1955. 

Toennies,  J.  F.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  i  :  378,  1938. 

Tomita,  T.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  6:  327,  1956. 

ToMiTA,  T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  245,  1957. 

Tomita,  T.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  7:  80,  1957. 

VoGT,  M.  J.  Physiol.  123:  451,  1954. 

von    Euler,    U.    S.    Noradrenaline.    Springfield:   Thomas, 

1956- 

Wilson,  V.  J.  and  D.  P.  C.  Lloyd.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  187: 

641,  1956. 


CHAPTER    VI 


Skeletal  neuromuscular  transmission 


PAULFATT     I     Biophysics  Department,  University  College,  London,  England 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Morphology 

Local  Electrical  Response 

Activity  of  the  Nerve  Terminals 

Properties  of  the  Junctional  Receptor 

Conclusion :  Mechanism  of  Transmission 


THE  EXISTENCE  OF  A  REGION  between  motor  nerve  and 
voluntary  muscle  which  has  special  properties 
emerged  from  experiments  on  the  action  of  the  South 
American  Indian  arrow  poison,  curare,  performed  lay 
Claude  Bernard  about  1850  (2).  Bernard's  aim  in  the 
first  place  was  to  show  that  muscle  was  excitable  inde- 
pendently of  its  nerve  supply.  Having;  pre\iously 
paralyzed  a  frog  with  curare,  he  isolated  a  nerve- 
muscle  preparation  and  showed  that,  while  an  elec- 
trical stimulus  applied  to  the  nerve  was  ineffective, 
a  contraction  resulted  if  it  were  applied  directly  to 
the  muscle.  Inferring  that  curare  interfered  with  the 
functioning  of  the  nerve  but  not  of  the  muscle,  he 
carried  the  investigation  a  step  further  by  preparing  a 
frog  with  a  ligature  which  interrupted  the  blood 
supply  to  the  hind  legs  but  not  the  nervous  connec- 
tions. When  curare  was  introduced  above  the  liga- 
ture, a  paralysis  developed  which  affected  only  the 
anterior  part  of  the  body.  Most  significant  was  the 
observation  that  pinching  the  skin  above  the  ligature 
did  not  elicit  movement  in  that  region  but  caused  the 
normal  reflex  thrust  of  the  hind  legs.  It  was  concluded 
from  this  that  curare  did  not  cause  a  loss  of  sensation, 
and  its  effect  was  therefore  ascribed  to  a  poisoning  of 
the  motor  nerve,  for,  as  was  already  seen,  in  the 
presence  of  curare  the  muscle  could  still  be  excited 
directly.  But  since  curare  apparently  did  not  affect 
the  motor  nerve  in  its  more  central  course  from  the 


spinal  cord  to  the  level  of  the  ligature  either,  it  wa 
maintained  that  the  poison  acted  on  the  motor  nerve 
only  in  its  most  peripheral  part,  where  contact  was 
made  with  the  muscle. 

Following  this  penetrating  study,  investigations 
were  carried  out  over  a  number  of  years  by  other 
workers  into  the  method  of  action  of  substances  that 
affect  nerve-muscle  transmission  more  or  less  specif- 
ically. Besides  curare,  one  of  the  chief  of  these  was 
nicotine.  When  a  small  amount  of  this  drug  was 
injected  into  an  animal  or  added  to  the  solution 
bathing  an  isolated  muscle,  a  contraction  occurred 
which  was  abolished  by  curare  at  the  same  time  as 
was  the  contraction  produced  by  nerve  stimulation. 
It  was  further  found  that  chronic  denervation  did  not 
eliminate  the  capacity  of  the  muscle  for  responding 
to  nicotine,  which  was  still  antagonized  by  curare, 
although  the  nerve  terminals  underwent  severe 
deterioration  (34,  46,  57,  59).  From  this  it  was  con- 
cluded that  the  site  of  action  of  curare,  as  well  as 
nicotine,  was  not  in  the  nerve  endings,  as  had  pre- 
viously been  supposed,  but  in  the  muscle  fiber. 

A  quantitative  investigation  of  the  effects  of  these 
substances  was  made  by  Langley  about  1910  (58, 
60,  61).  By  the  application  of  small  droplets  of  nicotine 
solution  along  a  mu.scle  fiber,  he  found  that  nicotine 
in  low  concentration  initiated  a  contraction  only  when 
applied  in  the  region  of  the  nerve  endings.  A  concen- 
tration one  thousand  times  greater  than  the  minimum 
effective  dose  was  required  to  produce  a  contraction 
elsewhere  along  the  muscle  fiber.  Furthermore, 
curare  interfered  with  the  action  of  nicotine  in  low 
concentration  but  had  no  effect  on  the  contraction 
produced  by  the  high  concentration  that  did  not  act 
exclusively  in  the  innervated  region.  The  manner  in 
which  curare  and  nicotine  acted  was  inferred  from  the 
observation  that  increasing  concentrations  of  curare 


'99 


200 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


were  able  to  antagonize  increasing  concentrations  of 
nicotine  over  a  wide  range  of  such  concentrations, 
and  that  the  effects  of  the  two  substances  were  to  some 
extent  reversible,  the  same  result  being  achieved 
irrespective  of  the  order  of  their  application.  This  led 
to  the  suggestion  that  nicotine  and  curare  competed 
with  each  other  in  forming  a  loose  combination  with 
a  'receptor  substance,'  which  was  thought  to  occur  in 
the  muscle  fiber  immediately  around  the  nerve 
endings  where  it  could  be  acted  upon  h\  the  nerve 
impulse.  Nicotine  or  the  nerve  impulse  when  acting 
on  this  receptor  would  lead  to  a  contraction,  while 
its  combination  with  curare  would  prevent  either  of 
them  being  effective. 

In  1936  the  concept  of  a  distinctive  chemical  process 
in  neuromuscular  transmission  was  given  a  secure 
foundation  by  the  work  of  Dale  and  his  followers. 
They  succeeded  in  showing  that  a  nerve  impulse  on 
reaching  the  terminals  in  a  muscle  caused  the  release 
of  a  pharmacologically  active  substance  (iS).  On  the 
repetitive  stimulation  of  the  motor  nerve  fibers,  to  the 
exclusion  of  other  types  of  nerve  fibers,  a  substance 
appeared  in  the  fluid  perfusing  the  muscle  that  was 
capable  of  causing  a  contraction  of  muscle  from  the 
leech  and  a  fall  in  arterial  pressure  of  the  cat.  From 
the  relative  effectiveness  of  the  substance  on  these  two 
test  preparations  and  the  modification  in  their 
response  produced  by  drugs,  as  well  as  from  the  chemi- 
cal stability  of  the  substance  under  various  condi- 
tions, it  was  concluded  to  be  acetylcholine,  the 
pharmacological  action  of  which  was  already  known. 
Its  release  was  found  to  be  undiminished  when  trans- 
mission was  abolished  by  curare.  Further  experi- 
ments showed  that  the  rapid  injection  of  acetylcholine 
into  a  muscle  by  its  blood  vessels  caused  the  excitation 
of  muscle  fibers  and  a  contraction  (7,  8,  9).  This 
occurred  in  the  chronically  denervated  muscle  as 
well  as  in  the  normal  one,  and,  as  in  the  case  of 
nicotine,  this  excitatory  action  could  be  abolished  by 
curare.  The  effect  of  physostigmine  was  also  ex- 
amined. It  was  found  to  prolong  and  intensify  the 
response  to  injected  acetylcholine  and  to  cause  repeti- 
tive muscle  discharges  to  a  single  nerve  impulse. 
From  earlier  studies  it  was  known  that  physostigmine 
has  the  specific  action  of  inhibiting  the  enzyme  that 
destroys  acetylcholine. 

All  these  findings  are  compatible  with  the  chemical 
theory  of  transmission,  according  to  which  transmis- 
sion is  accomplished  by  the  nerve  impulse  causing  the 
release  of  a  small  quantity  of  acetylcholine  from  the 
nerve  endings.  This  substance  combines  with  a  special 
receptor  substance   in   the  junctional   region   of  the 


muscle  fiber  and,  by  so  doing,  alters  the  properties 
of  the  fiber  in  such  a  way  as  to  lead  to  excitation  and 
contraction.  This  mediation  of  transmission  by  a 
specific  chemical  is  fundamentally  different  from  the 
process  occurring  when  an  impulse  is  conducted  along 
a  continuous  structure,  in  which  case  an  essential 
factor  for  the  spread  of  excitation  is  a  flow  of  electric 
current  between  adjacent  parts.  An  alternative 
explanation  of  neuromuscular  transmission  is  ex- 
pressed in  the  electrical  theory,  according  to  which 
transmission  is  affected  by  the  action  currents  gen- 
erated by  the  impulse  in  the  prejunctional  nerve 
terminals  passing  through  the  adjacent  muscle  fiber 
in  the  appropriate  direction  and  in  sufficient  magni- 
tude to  cause  excitation.  This  theory  was  formulated 
when  the  electrical  events  associated  with  the  con- 
ducted impulse  were  first  studied,  and  the  attempt 
was  made  to  account  for  both  processes  by  a  common 
mechanism.  The  selective  sensitivity  of  transmission  to 
various  treatments  was  ascriljed  to  secondary  effects, 
in  particular  to  the  alteration  of  the  electrical  ex- 
citability of  the  postjunctional  structure. 

The  results  of  experiments  in  which  chemicals  are 
involved,  either  the  collection  of  acetylcholine  after 
nerve  stimulation  or  the  application  of  various 
chemicals  to  evoke  or  modify  the  response  of  the 
postjunctional  structure,  are  consistent  with  the 
chemical  theory.  A  decisive  result  which  excludes  the 
possibility  of  electrical  transmission  comes  from  the 
study  of  the  alteration  of  properties  of  the  post- 
junctional region  during  transmission.  It  is  found  that 
the  characteristic  alteration  responsible  for  excitation 
of  the  muscle  fiber  cannot  be  brought  about  by  a 
current  generated  externally  to  the  fiber.  On  the  other 
hand  an  alteration  of  precisely  this  type  is  produced 
by  the  application  of  acetylcholine  to  the  junctional 
region  of  the  muscle  fiber.  Accepting  the  correctness 
of  the  chemical  theory  of  transmission,  one  is  able  to 
give  an  integrated  account  of  a  wide  range  of  experi- 
mental observation,  distinguishing  between  those 
events  which  occur  prejunctionally  and  involve  the 
release  of  acetylcholine,  and  those  which  occur  post- 
junctionally  and  involve  the  reaction  of  acetylcholine 
with  the  receptor  and  the  resultant  change  in  the 
properties  of  the  muscle  fiber  membrane. 


MORPHOLOGY 

The  detailed  morphological  description  which  fol- 
lows applies  to  junctions  on  skeletal  muscle  in  verte- 
brates where  the  normal  response  to  a  single  nerve 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


201 


impulse  is  a  propagated  action  potential  and  a  twitch. 
These  are  the  junctions  of  which  both  the  morphology 
and  physiology  have  been  most  intensively  studied. 
There  are  other  junctions,  in  the  amphibian  at  least, 
where  the  normal  mechanical  response  of  the  muscle 
fibers  is  a  slow  tonic  contraction  which  can  only  be 
elicited  in  appreciable  tension  by  a  train  of  nerve 
impulses  (53,  55).  These  fibers  are  innervated  by  a 
special  class  of  small  diameter  nerve  fibers  which  form 
numerous,  widely  distributed  terminations  of  the 
en  grappe  type  on  them. 

The  twitch  muscle  fibers  are  innervated  by  coarse 
motor  nerve  fibers.  On  issuing  from  the  central 
nervous  system,  each  nerve  fiber  branches  repeatedly 
both  before  and  after  reaching  the  muscle  it  supplies. 
By  this  branching  the  nerve  fiber  forms  junctions  on 
many  muscle  fibers,  the  number  varying  greatly  for 
mu.scles  in  different  parts  of  the  body  in  a  given 
animal.  Conversely,  muscle  fibers  have  been  found 
to  be  supplied  each  with  a  few  nerve  endings  at 
widely  separated  positions  along  their  length  (47,  50). 
These  multiple  junctions  are  in  some  cases  made  by 
separate  nerve  fibers  and  in  others  bv  branches  of  a 
single  fiber.  The  variations  in  the  distribution  of  nerve 
fibers  to  muscle  fibers  in  different  preparations  and 
their  probable  relation  to  differences  in  function  have 
been  discussed  by  Tiegs  (73). 

In  the  morphology  of  the  single  junction,  the  pat- 
tern made  by  the  nerve  fiber  in  terminating  also 
shows  marked  differences  from  species  to  species  and 
from  muscle  to  muscle.  This  field  was  early  thoroughly 
explored  Ijy  Kiihne  (56).  Confining  consideration  to 
the  more  familiar  objects  of  investigation,  he  drew  a 
distinction  between  the  plate  type  of  ending  in  the 
mammal  and  reptile  and  the  bush  type  in  the  frog.  In 
both  types  the  nerve  comes  into  contact  with  the 
muscle  fiber  immediately  after  losing  its  myelin 
sheath  and  branches  repeatedly  on  its  surface  to  form 
the  terminal  apparatus.  In  the  former  type,  the 
extent  of  this  apparatus  is  limited  to  a  roughly  circular 
space  (the  endplate)  which  has  a  diameter  of  25  to 
70  /x.  Viewed  in  a  section  at  right  angles  to  the  muscle 
fiber  surface  this  region  is  marked  by  a  rounded 
eminence.  Within  the  confines  of  the  endplate  the 
terminal  branches  cover  a  large  fraction  of  the  in- 
cluded muscle  fiber  surface.  In  the  case  of  the  other 
type  of  ending  (the  endbush)  the  nerve  terminal 
branches  range  over  a  much  wider  area.  The  terminal 
apparatus  here  consists  mainly  of  several  large, 
straight  branches  100  to  300  /j.  in  length,  running 
parallel  to  the  axis  of  the  muscle  fiber  and  connected 


into  a  continuous  system  by  shorter  lengths  at  right 
angles. 

As  a  result  of  careful  cytological  examination,  it  is 
recognized  that  there  are  three  sharply  defined  com- 
ponents of  different  cellular  origin  at  the  junction 
(14).  The  first  of  these  is  the  terminal  apparatus  of  the 
nerve.  The  second  is  the  specialized  region  of  muscle 
fiber  surface  contacted  by  the  nerve  endings.  A  char- 
acteristic of  this  region  is  an  increased  density  of 
muscle  nuclei  (fundamental  nuclei  of  the  junction), 
the  presence  of  which  is  suggestive  of  a  higher  degree 
of  synthetic  activity  here  than  elsewhere  in  the  muscle 
fiber.  The  third  component  is  a  layer  of  neuroglia 
which  in  this  position  is  referred  to  as  the  teloglia 
and  which  appear  to  be  continuous  with  the  -Schwann 
cell  envelope  of  the  myelinated  fiber.  It  contributes 
about  half  the  nuclei  seen  in  the  junctional  region 
(the  sole  nuclei),  the  remainder  being  the  fundamental 
nuclei  in  the  muscle.  It  is  dispersed  over  the  entire 
endplate  where  it  forms  the  rounded  eminence  and 
accompanies  the  terminal  nerve  filaments  along  their 
extended  course  in  the  endbush.  In  spite  of  the  gross 
differences  that  exist  between  the  two  types  of  ending, 
the  detailed  relationships  between  these  three  cellular 
components  are  fundamentally  the  same.  From  the  use 
of  cytological  and  histochemical  staining  methods  it 
appears  that  the  nerve  terminal  branches  lie  sunk  in 
grooves  in  the  muscle  fiber  surface  (14,  16).  Only  a 
small  part  of  the  circumference  of  the  nerve  is  not  in 
close  proximity  to  the  surface  of  the  muscle  lining  the 
groove.  The  sides  of  the  groove  appear  to  be  marked 
with  a  set  of  parallel  lines  0.3  to  i  m  apart  which  are 
oriented  more  or  less  normal  to  the  axis  of  the  groove 
and  extend  a  short  distance  into  the  muscle  beyond 
the  clearly  defined  edges  of  the  nerve  cylinder.  In  the 
case  of  the  endbush,  where  there  are  long  stretches  of 
unbranched  nerve  fiber,  the  ruling  is  highly  regular, 
the  lines  running  from  one  edge  of  the  groove  to  the 
other  without  deviating  from  this  geometrical  rela- 
tion. In  the  endplate  where  the  nerve  filaments 
usually  extend  for  no  more  than  a  few  diameters 
before  terminating  or  branching,  the  arrangement 
of  the  lines  is  less  regular,  adjacent  lines  frequently 
fuse  with  one  another,  while  their  spacing  is  main- 
tained relatively  constant.  Examination  of  the  junc- 
tion with  the  electron  microscope  reveals  regularly 
spaced  narrow  infoldings  of  the  membrane  of  the 
muscle  fiber  lining  the  groove  (71).  These  fine  junc- 
tional folds  very  probably  correspond  to  the  lines 
seen  under  the  light  microscope.  Figures  i  and  2  show 
the  relation  between  nerve  and  muscle  over  a  wide 
range   of  magnification.   The   width   of  the   folds   is 


202  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NELfROPHYSIOLOG\'    I 


Wfff^ 


5m 


FIG.  1.  Surface  view  of  neuromuscular  junction  of  lizarcl  stained  with  Janus  green.  The  only 
parts  to  have  taken  up  the  stain  are  the  regions  of  muscle  bordering  the  ner\e  terminals  (the  sub- 
neural  apparatus)  and  a  short  piece  of  nerve  at  the  termination  of  the  myelin.  The  final  part  of 
the  myelinated  nerve  fiber  appears  in  the  extreme  left  of  the  upper  picture.  In  the  lower  picture  a 
portion  of  the  junction  is  shown  at  higher  magnification  revealing  the  lines  in  the  subneural  appara- 
tus, which  are  oriented  at  right  angles  to  the  edge  of  the  nerve  and  which  are  uniformly  spaced 
about  0.4  M  apart.  [From  Couteau.x  (15).] 

FIG.  2.  Electronmicrograph  of  lizard  neuromuscular  junction.  Two  nerve  terminal  branches  are 
seen  in  the  left  side  of  the  main  picture  with  the  muscle  to  the  right.  The  dark  oval  bodies  in  the 
nerve  and  muscle  are  mitochondria.  The  surface  of  the  muscle  at  the  junction  is  thrown  into  a 
series  of  folds,  which  correspond  in  their  repetition  interval  and  depth  to  the  lines  in  hg.  i .  From 
the  appearance  where  the  surface  membrane  of  the  nerve  can  be  clearly  seen,  it  is  established  that 
it  does  not  enter  the  folds.  The  inset  gives  an  enlarged  view  of  the  situation  at  the  junction.  The 
surface  membranes  of  nerve  and  muscle  probably  correspond  to  the  two  dense  lines  separated  by 
about  0.07  fi.  [From  Robertson  (71)] 


about  0.05  /i  and  their  depth  about  0.5  ^l.  This  in- 
folding considerably  increases  the  area  of  postjunc- 
tional membrane  which  may  have  an  important 
bearing  on  the  magnitude  of  the  alteration  produced 
in  the  junctional  region  during  transmission.  The 
teloglia  does  not  occur  within  the  grooves  but  appears 
to  remain  in  contact  with  the  exposed  part  of  the  nerve 
cylinder.  This  suggests  that  it  plays  no  direct  role  in 
the  transmission  process. 


LOCAL    ELECTRICAL    RESPONSE 

The  study  of  neuromuscular  transmission  received 
a  great  impetus  with  the  application  of  electrical 
recording  techniques  to  the  junctional  region.  It 
was  observed  by  a  number  of  workers  that  after  a 
muscle  had  been  treated  with  just  sufficient  curare  to 


prevent  contraction  from  nerve  stimulation,  there 
still  occurred  an  electrical  change  in  the  muscle, 
though  this  was  different  from  the  action  potential 
type  of  response  (13,  30,  33,  43,  45,  72).  The  response 
was  not  propagated,  being  recorded  in  monophasic 
form  between  different  positions  along  the  muscle.  In 
the  sartorius  muscle  of  the  frog,  with  one  electrode 
kept  on  the  nerve-free  pelvic  end  and  the  other  moved 
from  place  to  place,  the  magnitude  of  the  recorded 
potential  change  was  found  to  be  correlated  with  the 
density  of  nerve  endings  under  the  moving  electrode. 
The  potential  change  arising  at  a  focus  of  nerve 
endings  (recorded  with  respect  to  a  distant  nerve-free 
point  on  the  muscle)  consists  of  a  transient  negative 
deflection  having  a  relatively  brief  rising  phase  and  a 
slower  return,  the  later  part  of  which  follows  an 
approximately  exponential  time  course.  This  response 
has  been  generally  referred   to  as  the  endplate  po- 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


203 


tential,  noivvithstanding  that  in  the  amphibian 
muscle,  where  it  has  been  studied  most,  the  nerve 
ending  is  not  of  the  morphological  form  described  as 
an  endplate. 

On  increasing  the  concentration  of  curare  in  the 
fluid  bathing  the  muscle,  the  amplitude  of  the  response 
is  reduced.  When,  on  the  other  hand,  the  concentra- 
tion is  decreased  from  that  required  to  block  transmis- 
sion, action  potentials  in  individual  muscle  fibers 
appear  as  more  rapid  and  complex  deflections  super- 
imposed on  the  endplate  potential.  With  further 
reduction  of  curare,  the  action  potential  component 
increases  and  obscures  the  endplate  potential.  The 
endplate  potential  was  early  inferred  to  be  developed 
across  the  surface  membrane  of  the  muscle  fiber, 
although  confined  to  its  junctional  region,  because  of 
the  similarity  of  this  potential  with  that  which  could 
be  evoked  by  a  brief  pulse  of  current  applied  any- 
where along  the  muscle.  More  compelling  evidence 
came  from  the  study  of  the  interaction  of  the  junc- 
tional response  and  the  muscle  action  potential,  the 
latter  elicited  by  direct  stimulation  and  propagated 
into  the  junctional  region.  It  was  found  by  this 
method  that  the  action  potential  and  the  endplate 
potential  did  not  sum  with  each  other,  and  that  the 
action  potential  was  capable  of  aboli.shing  the  later 
part  of  the  endplate  potential  when  timed  to  coincide 
with  its  summit. 

The  most  accurate  basis  for  an  analysis  of  the 
potential  changes  in  the  muscle  fiber  to  determine  the 
manner  of  their  generation  is  the  results  from  intra- 
cellular recording  (40).  This  involves  inserting  a  very 
fine  electrode  through  the  surface  membrane  of 
individual  muscle  fibers  and  recording  potentials 
between  it  and  another  electrode  in  the  surrounding 
fluid.  Intracellular  recording  not  only  makes  more 
accurate  measurements  of  the  electrical  response 
possible  but  also  greatly  simplifies  its  interpretation. 
After  minor  corrections  for  extracellular  gradients  of 
potential  when  current  is  flowing,  the  potentials 
observed  by  this  method  are  those  obtaining  across  the 
surface  membrane  of  the  muscle  fiber  at  the  position 
of  insertion  of  the  electrode.  The  frog  muscle  fiber  is 
found  to  have  a  resting  membrane  potential  of 
about  90  mv  (inside  negative  with  respect  to  outside), 
which  is  the  same  in  the  junctional  region  as  elsewhere 
along  the  fiber.  The  addition  of  curare  to  the  solution 
bathing  the  muscle  in  a  concentration  sufiicient  to 
block  transmission  has  no  effect  on  this  resting  po- 
tential. With  the  intracellular  electrode  situated  in 
the  junctional  region  of  the  fiber,  an  endplate  po- 
tential is  recorded  in  response  to  nerve  stimulation. 


It  appears  as  a  transient  positive  deflection,  i.e.  as  a 
transient  reduction  of  membrane  potential  from  its 
resting  level.  Its  amplitude  varies  from  fiber  to  fiber 
and  depends  upon  the  concentration  of  curare.  In  a 
frog  sartorius  muscle,  critically  curarized  to  abolish 
contraction,  different  fibers  have  been  found  to  dis- 
play endplate  potentials  ranging  from  i  mv  to  more 
than  20  mv.  The  response  would  be  expected  under 
these  conditions  to  range  in  size  up  to  the  threshold 
depolarization  for  initiating  an  action  potential 
which  would  be  about  40  mv.  Immediatelv  at  the 
junction  the  endplate  potential  has  a  rising  phase 
lasting  1.5  msec.  Following  the  attainment  of  the 
summit,  the  potential  declines  to  one  half  in  another 
2  msec.  The  rate  of  fractional  decay  decreases  beyond 
this,  the  time  required  to  fall  from  one  half  to  one 
quarter  being  about  5  m.sec.  A  potential  change  can  be 
detected  at  points  on  the  fiber  up  to  a  few  millimeters 
distant  from  the  nerve  ending,  becoming  progressively 
more  attenuated  and  slowed  with  increasing  distance 

(fig-  3)- 

This  potential  wave  has  been  analyzed  to  determine 
the  movement  of  charge  underlying  it.  The  amplitude 
of  the  potential  at  various  instants  is  plotted  against 
distance  along  the  fiber.  Assuming  that  the  membrane 
capacity  remains  constant  during  the  response,  the 
curves  thus  formed  indicate  the  spatial  distribution  of 
charge  displaced  from  the  membrane  capacity  (rela- 
tive to  its  initial  condition  of  charge).  The  area 
beneath  each  curve  is  a  measure  of  the  total  charge 
displaced  at  the  given  instant.  The  plot  of  these  areas 
against  time  shows  that  the  charge  is  built  up  to  a 
maximum  in  about  2  msec,  and  after  this  it  decays 
exponentially  with  a  time  constant  of  about  25  msec. 
A  determination  of  the  passive  electrical  characteris- 
tics of  the  muscle  fiber  shows  that  this  latter  value 
corresponds  to  the  electric  time  constant  of  the  mem- 
brane. This  result  is  consistent  with  the  idea  that 
there  is  a  brief  phase  of  transmitter  action,  confined  to 
about  the  initial  2  msec,  of  the  response,  during  which 
charge  is  transferred  inward  across  the  membrane, 
and  that  this  is  followed  by  a  gradual  dissipation  of  the 
displaced  charge  at  a  rate  determined  by  the  electrical 
characteristics  of  the  inactive  fiber  membrane.  It 
agrees  with  the  results  of  the  interaction  between  the 
endplate  potential  and  action  potential  from  which  it 
appears  that  the  charge  displacement  built  up  by 
junctional  activity  can  be  removed  by  the  high  con- 
ductance of  the  spike  at  a  time  shortly  following  the 
summit  of  the  endplate  potential. 

From  a  knowledge  of  the  membrane  capacity  for  a 
imit  length  of  fil)er,  the  displacement  of  charge  may 


204 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


be  calculated.  For  an  endplate  potential  of  20  mv 
peak  amplitude  the  maximum  displacement  of  charge 
is  found  to  be  about  io~'  coulombs.  Given  informa- 
tion on  the  complete  electrical  characteristics  of  the 
fiber,  i.e.  of  the  separate  values  of  membrane  capacity 
and  membrane  and  core  conductances,  it  is  possible 
to  analyze  more  completely  the  potential  wave  in 
the  fiber.  An  approximate  treatment,  in  which  the 
observed  response  of  the  fiber  is  compared  with  a 
theoretically  derived  potential  wave  for  charge  placed 
instantaneously  at  a  point  along  the  fiber,  confirms 
the  above  interpretation  of  the  generation  of  the  end- 
plate  response  by  a  brief  transfer  of  charge  in  a  small 
area  of  membrane. 

In  a  normal  uncurarized  muscle,  the  rate  of  rise  of 
the  endplate  potential  is  about  three  times  as  fast  as 
in  the  above  case,  due  evidently  to  a  proportionately 
more  intense  transfer  of  charge.  On  the  endplate 
potential  reaching  a  level  of  depolarization  of  about 
40  mv,  an  action  potential  is  initiated,  indicated  by  a 
sudden  increase  in  the  rate  of  change  of  potential. 
The  threshold  for  the  initiation  of  an  action  potential 
has  Ijeen  examined  by  the  direct  application  of  a 
current  pulse,  both  at  the  junction  and  away  from  it, 
and  has  been  found  to  occur  at  all  points  at  this  same 
level.  The  spike  which  follows  the  initial  depolariza- 
tion produced  by  the  endplate  potential  is  however 
characteristically  different  at  the  junction  where  it 
is  evoked  from  elsewhere  in  the  course  of  its  propaga- 
tion (40,  69;  cf.  fig.  4}.  After  rising  from  the  level  of 
threshold  to  zero  membrane  potential  at  a  rate  which 


is  not  noticeably  different  in  the  two  cases,  the 
junctional  spike  produces  a  smaller  reversal  of  mem- 
brane potential  than  the  normal  muscle  spike  away 
from  the  junction.  Thus,  at  the  summit  of  the  junc- 
tional spike  the  membrane  potential  is  reversed  to  the 
extent  of  about  20  mv  (total  spike  height  of  1 1  o  mv), 
compared  to  a  reversal  of  about  35  mv  for  the  normal 
spike  (total  height,  125  mv).  The  summit  of  the 
junctional  spike  occurs  earlier  and  is  sharper  than 
that  of  the  normal  spike.  After  reaching  the  summit 
the  potential  falls  to  the  level  of  zero  membrane 
potential  where  it  remains  nearly  steady  for  about 
1.5  msec,  before  declining  further.  In  contrast,  the 
normal  spike  declines  rather  slowly  for  about  2 
msec,  after  its  summit,  but  then  falls  more  rapidly 
past  zero  membrane  potential. 

It  can  be  shown  that  these  features,  which  distin- 
guish the  junctional  spike,  do  not  depend  on  some 
special  characteristic  of  the  action  potential  process  in 
the  region  where  it  occurs.  When  an  action  potential 
is  propagated  into  the  junctional  region  without  the 
nerve  having  been  active,  the  response  is  the  normal 
muscle  action  potential  similar  to  that  which  is  elicited 
elsewhere  along  the  fiber.  Moreover,  these  features 
cannot  be  attributed  to  the  response  having  originated 
in  the  region  of  observation  rather  than  having 
propagated  into  it,  since  the  propagated  action 
potential  and  the  one  which  is  initiated  in  the  region 
of  recording  by  a  brief  pulse  of  current  show  little 
difference  beyond  the  attainment  of  threshold.  It  is 
concluded  therefore  that  these  features  arise  from  a 


10  msec 


FIG.  3.  Endplate  potentials  recorded  intracellularly  from  a  single  curarized  muscle  fiber  of  a 
frog.  The  series  of  five  records  were  taken  at  intervals  of  i  mm  along  the  fiber.  The  top  record  shows 
the  response  at  the  junction  as  inferred  from  the  fact  that  the  response  was  maximum  at  this  posi- 
tion. [From  Fatt  &  Katz  (38).] 

FIG.  4.  Action  potentials  recorded  in  a  muscle  fiber  in  response  to  a  nerve  impulse.  The  upper 
record  was  taken  at  the  junction,  the  location  of  which  had  earlier  been  determined  by  the  response 
in  the  presence  of  curare.  The  lower  record  was  taken  2.5  mm  away  in  the  same  fiber.  A  trace  of 
the  endplate  potential  can  still  be  seen  in  the  lower  record,  appearing  as  a  gradual  rise  of  potential 
which  precedes  the  foot  of  the  spike.  [From  Fatt  &  Katz  (38).] 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


20  n 


modification  of  the  action  potential  response  by 
junctional  activity.  The  effect  of  this  activity  is  con- 
sistently to  cause  a  deviation  toward  a  level  near 
zero  membrane  potential.  This  accounts  for  the  reduc- 
tion in  peak  amplitude  of  the  spike  and  the  delay  on 
the  falling  phase.  As  a  first  approximation  it  may  be 
assumed  that  the  fundamental  changes  effected  in 
the  membrane  by  the  two  types  of  activity  which  are 
superimposed  at  the  junction  (i.e.  spike  and  junctional 
activity)  do  not  interact.  The  effect  of  transmitter 
action  on  the  spike,  as  well  as  the  initial  development 
of  the  endplate  potential,  can  then  be  satisfactorily 
accounted  for  by  an  increase  in  membrane  conduct- 
ance in  series  with  an  emf  set  near  the  level  of  zero 
membrane  potential.  However  during  the  action 
potential  the  situation  is  complicated  by  the  presence 
in  the  membrane  of  two  important  components  of 
conductance,  one  due  to  the  passage  of  sodium  ions 
and  the  other  to  potassium  ions,  which  follow  different 
time  courses  and  are  dependent  on  the  level  of  mem- 
brane potential.  In  order  to  determine  the  effect  of 
transmitter  action  more  accurately,  the  spike  has  been 
set  up  independentlv  of  the  nerve  response  by  direct 
stimulation  of  the  muscle  fiber  (24).  In  this  way,  using 
a  suitably  timed  nerve  impulse,  transinitter  action  was 
made  to  begin  at  any  chosen  stage  of  the  action  po- 
tential process,  and  the  resultant  deviation  of  the 
potential  observed.  It  was  thus  shown  that  the 
equilibrium  potential  for  junctional  activity  lies 
between  10  and  20  mv,  with  the  interior  of  the  fiber 
negative. 

The  generation  of  the  endplate  potential  has  also 
been  studied  in  the  absence  of  an  action  potential  by 
applying  a  steady  current  to  the  muscle  fiber  and 
thereby  altering  the  membrane  potential  at  which 
the  transmitter  operates.  .Significant  results  have  been 
obtained  only  with  currents  directed  inwardly  across 
the  membrane  and  causing  a  hyperpolarization, 
since  with  currents  in  the  opposite  directions  complica- 
tions arise  owing  to  the  initiation  of  muscle  action 
potentials.  The  endplate  potential  was  found  to  vary 
in  such  a  manner  as  to  maintain  its  rate  of  rise  nearly 
directly  proportional  to  the  level  of  membrane  po- 
tential at  which  it  occurred.  An  equally  good  fit  of 
the  data  could  be  obtained  with  a  straight  line  for 
which  zero  respon.se  would  occur  at  a  membrane 
potential  of  15  mv,  internally  negative.  There  is  thus 
complete  agreement,  as  far  as  the  equilibrium  value 
is  concerned,  between  the  results  obtained  from  the 
effect  of  junctional  activity  on  the  membrane  at  rest 
and  on  the  membrane  undergoing  an  action  po- 
tential. 


In  the  case  of  the  endplate  potential  arising  in  the 
otherwise  resting  membrane,  an  analysis  has  been 
made  to  determine  what  size  the  added  conductance 
would  have  to  be  to  produce  the  observed  rising  phase 
of  the  response.  The  muscle  filler  has  been  treated  as 
a  cable  with  known  distributive  characteristics,  and 
the  conductance  has  been  considered  as  applied 
suddenly  at  a  point  along  this  cable.  From  the  change 
of  potential  occurring  in  the  uncurarized  muscle  up 
to  the  level  at  which  the  spike  is  initiated,  the  con- 
ductance is  calculated  to  correspond  to  a  resistance 
of  about  20,000  ohms.  This  may  be  considered  in 
relation  to  the  resting  resistance  of  about  500,000 
ohms,  which  is  shunted  as  a  result  of  junctional 
activity,  and  which  is  in  effect  the  resistance  of  the 
membrane  over  a  length  of  about  4  mm  of  fiber  (twice 
the  space  constant  of  the  fiijer).  An  analysis  has  also 
been  made  of  the  effect  of  junctional  activity  to  reduce 
the  reversal  of  membrane  potential  at  the  .summit  of 
the  spike,  together  with  any  additional  displacement 
produced  by  an  applied  current.  The  added  conduct- 
ance calculated  from  this  information  is  roughly  in 
agreement  with  the  value  obtained  from  the  rising 
phase  of  the  endplate  potential. 

There  appears  thus  to  be  a  convergence  of  evidence 
to  show  that  the  effect  of  junctional  activity  on  the 
muscle  fiber  membrane  can  be  represented  as  the 
addition  of  a  conductance  in  scries  with  a  fixed  emf 
This  may  further  be  interpreted  as  the  creation  of  a 
new  path  for  the  diffusion  of  ions  across  the  mem- 
brane. The  equilibrium  value  (15  mv,  internally 
negative)  toward  which  the  membrane  potential  is 
displaced  is  the  same  as  the  emf  that  would  be 
expected  to  occur  for  the  unrestricted  diffusion  of  ions 
between  two  solutions,  having  the  ionic  composition 
of  the  intra-  and  extracellular  media.  It  is  therefore 
concluded  that  in  the  new  diffusion  path  created  by 
transmitter  action,  no  selectivity  is  exerted  in  the 
passage  of  different  ion  species  other  than  that  already 
existing  in  the  aqueous  media  on  the  two  sides  of  the 
membrane. 

The  investigations  on  neuromuscidar  transmission 
considered  so  far  in  this  section  have  concerned  the 
amphibian  muscle  fibers  that  under  normal  conditions 
respond  to  a  nerve  impulse  with  a  twitch.  The  con- 
clusions reached,  as  to  the  fundamental  alteration  in 
the  postjunctional  membrane  produced  by  the  action 
of  the  transmitter,  seem  likely  to  be  valid  generally  for 
junctions  on  vertebrate  skeletal  muscle  fibers.  How- 
ever, marked  variations  in  the  overall  electrical 
response  have  been  found  to  occur  in  different  prepa- 
rations, and  these  are  adduced  to  stem  mainly  from 


206 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


differences  in  the  electrical  characteristics  of  the 
muscle  fiber  membrane  in  parallel  with  the  junc- 
tionally  responding  region. 

In  the  mammalian  muscle  fiber  under  normal 
conditions,  the  endplate  potential  does  not  form  a 
conspicuous  step  on  the  rising  phase  of  the  spike  as  it 
does  in  the  amphibian  (4).  The  explanation  for  this 
lies  in  the  fact  that  the  threshold  depolarization  for 
initiating  an  action  potential  is  here  much  lower 
(about  10  mv  compared  with  40  mv  in  the  frog),  and 
at  this  level  the  transition  between  endplate  potential 
and  spike  does  not  involve  an  appreciable  change  in 
rate  of  rise  of  potential.  In  the  curarized  preparation 
the  response  differs  from  that  seen  in  the  frog  in  hav- 
ing a  shorter  decaying  phase  and  in  becoming  at- 
tenuated more  rapidly  with  increasing  distance  from 
the  junction.  These  differences  are  attributable 
entirely  to  a  higher  conductance  of  the  muscle  fiber 
membrane  with  a  consequent  reduction  in  the  electric 
time  and  space  constants. 

The  tonic  muscle  fibers  of  the  frog,  supplied  by  the 
small  diameter  motor  nerve  fibers,  display  differences 
in  their  electrical  response  from  the  twitch  fibers  of 
the  same  animal  which  are  again  mainly  attributable 
to  the  electrical  properties  of  the  fiber  membrane, 
though  the  disposition  of  the  nerve  endings  also  plays 
an  important  part  (10,  11,  54).  The  tonic  muscle 
fiber  is  unable  to  develop  an  action  potential,  due 
apparently  to  the  absence  of  the  mechanism  by  which 
the  sodium  permeability  of  the  membrane  is  increased 
by  depolarization.  The  entire  time  course  of  the 
junctional  response  can  therefore  be  observed  under 
all  conditions  without  the  complication  of  a  super- 
imposed spike.  In  addition  the  amplitude  of  the  junc- 
tional response  can  be  varied  by  stimulation  of  differ- 
ent nerve  fibers.  Owing  to  a  wide  and  relatively 
uniform  distribution  of  their  endings  along  the  muscle 
fiber,  the  potential  wave  does  not  show  a  marked 
attenuation  with  distance,  and  at  no  position  does  it 
have  the  initially  rapid  and  later  relatively  slow 
decline  of  the  endplate  potential  recorded  at  the 
junction  of  a  twitch  fiber.  Another  distinctive  feature 
of  the  response  in  these  fibers  is  that  the  membrane 
potential  goes  through  a  phase  of  hyperpolarization 
after  recovering  from  the  depolarization.  A  similar 
phase  of  hyperpolarization  is  found  to  follow  a  wave 
of  depolarization  elicited  by  a  current  pulse  applied 
directly  to  the  muscle  fiber,  from  which  it  is  inferred 
that  this  feature  is  not  due  to  .some  peculiar  charac- 
teristic of  the  transmission  process  but  depends  rather 
on  the  electrical  behavior  of  the  membrane. 


.\CT1VITY  OF  THE  NERVE  TERMIN.^LS 

In  the  preceding  .section,  the  local  electrical  changes 
brought  about  in  the  normal  and  in  the  curarized 
muscle  fiber  by  the  arrival  of  an  impulse  in  the  pre- 
junctional nerve  terminals  have  been  described.  In 
this  section  the  behavior  of  the  terminals  will  be  con- 
sidered under  various  conditions,  in  .so  far  as  this 
throws  light  on  their  specialized  properties.  Almost 
all  the  information  to  be  presented  is  derived  from 
recording  potential  changes  in  the  muscle  fiber. 
According  to  the  chemical  theory  of  transmission, 
activity  in  the  nerve  terminals  causes  a  release  of 
acetylcholine  which  then  reacts  with  the  muscle  to 
produce  an  alteration  in  it.  Hence,  when  recording 
from  the  muscle,  an  indication  of  activity  at  the 
terminals  is  obtained,  provided  that  allowance  is 
made  for  possible  effects  in  the  later  stages  of  the 
transmission  process.  An  example  of  such  an  effect  is 
the  reduction  of  the  responsiveness  of  the  muscle 
fiber  by  curare  through  its  competition  with  acetyl- 
choline. 

When  the  membrane  potential  is  recorded  in  the 
junctional  region  of  a  muscle  fiber,  a  sequence  of 
small  transient  changes  of  potential  (as  shown  in  fig. 
5)  is  observed  even  in  the  absence  of  a  nerve  impulse 
(3,  41,  62).  Although  their  peak  amplitude  is  only  of 
the  order  of  0.5  mv,   these  potential  changes  have 


2raV 


50  msec 


FIG.  5.  Spontaneously  occurring  miniature  endplate  poten- 
tials recorded  at  the  junctional  region  of  a  muscle  liber  of  a 
frog.  The  location  of  the  recording  position  was  confirmed  by 
the  form  of  the  response  elicited  by  a  nerve  impulse.  [From 
Fatt  &  Katz  (39).] 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


207 


many  of  the  characteristics  of  a  response  to  a  nerve 
impulse.  Their  time  course  is  similar  to  the  endplate 
potential  in  a  curarized  muscle.  They  appear  largest 
at  the  same  place  along  the  muscle  fiber  and  become 
attenuated  by  changes  in  the  position  of  the  recording 
electrode  in  the  same  way.  Furthermore,  they  are 
diminished  in  amplitude  by  curare  and  increased 
and  prolonged  by  anticholinesterases.  All  the.se  fea- 
tures may  be  accounted  for  by  the  properties  of  the 
postjunctional  element  and  its  reaction  with  acetyl- 
choline. That  the  nerve  terminals  are  responsible  for 
the  release  of  acetylcholine  producing  these  dis- 
charges— called  miniature  endplate  potentials — is 
shown  by  the  fact  that  they  are  abolished  on  nerve 
degeneration  and  their  frequency  of  occurrence  is 
modified  by  various  treatments  applied  to  the  nerve. 
In  addition  there  is  strong  evidence  that  the  end- 
plate potential  evoked  by  a  nerve  impulse  is  itself 
resolvable  into  units  of  the  size  of  miniature  poten- 
tials. 

The  miniature  discharges  occur  in  a  random  time 
sequence,  the  probability  of  occurrence  in  any  given 
inter\'al  of  time  remaining  constant  irrespective  of 
previous  discharges.  The  distribution  of  intervals  be- 
tween successive  discharges  is  accordingly  found  to 
follow  a  simple  exponential  function,  decaying  with 
increasing  interval,  and  can  be  descril)ed  by  a  single 
parameter,  the  mean  frequency  of  discharge.  E.xcep- 
tions  to  this  are  occasional  bursts  which  consist  of  a 
number  of  miniature  endplate  potentials  occurring 
within  a  short  period  of  time.  They  are  the  only  indi- 
cation of  a  possible  coupling  between  discharges,  and 
can  be  readily  recognized  and  excluded  from  a  sta- 
tistical analysis.  In  the  frog  under  normal  conditions 
the  mean  frequency  of  spontaneous  discharges  varies 
greatly  at  different  junctions,  extending  at  least  over 
the  range  o.  i  per  sec.  to  100  per  .sec.  In  mammalian 
muscle  the  frequency  is  more  nearly  constant  around 
I  per  sec. 

The  distribution  of  amplitudes  of  the  miniature 
endplate  potentials  at  a  junction  can  be  fitted  ap- 
proximately by  a  Gaussian  curve  with  a  standard 
deviation  equal  to  about  30  per  cent  of  the  mean. 
With  this  relatively  small  variation,  the  amplitudes 
effectively  do  not  grade  down  to  zero,  and  hence 
under  suitable  recording  conditions  there  is  no  un- 
certainty in  counting  the  discharges.  By  a  variation  in 
recording  technique,  placing  the  microelectrode  in 
contact  with  the  muscle  fiber  membrane  without 
penetrating  it,  it  is  possible  to  restrict  the  recording 
of  miniature  discharges  to  those  arising  in  a  small 
fraction   of  the  junctional   region   contacted   by   the 


nerve  terminals.  In  this  way,  one  tenth  or  so  of  the 
miniature  discharges  occurring  within  the  fiber  are 
recorded  selectively  while  the  remainder  appear 
greatly  attenuated  and  are  in  eflPect  rejected  (27). 
Even  under  these  conditions  the  amplitude  of  the 
miniature  potentials  appears  to  be  continuously 
distributed,  there  being  no  clear  indication  of  a 
number  of  discrete  .sizes  which  are  repeated. 

A  notable  feature  of  the  miniature  discharge  is 
that  the  release  of  acetylcholine  which  produces  it 
does  not  appear  to  change  under  various  treatments 
which  have  an  important  influence  on  the  genera- 
tion of  an  electrical  response  (28).  Even  in  the  situa- 
tion where  the  nerve  and  muscle  membranes  have 
been  completely  depolarized  i)y  a  high  concentra- 
tion of  potassiimi  ions,  it  can  be  shown  by  repolarizing 
the  muscle  fiber  with  an  applied  current  that  the 
intermittent  release  of  sinall  quantities  of  acetyl- 
choline, capable  of  producing  miniature  potentials, 
still  occurs  (26).  It  is  therefore  concluded  that  the 
release  of  acetylcholine  forming  these  discharges  does 
not  depend  upon  the  occurrence  of  electrical  activity 
of  the  action  potential  type  in  any  structural  unit 
within  the  nerve  terminal. 

Unlike  the  amplitude  (considered  as  a  quantity  of 
acetylcholine  released  from  the  terminal),  the  fre- 
quency of  the  spontaneous  discharges  is  highly  sensi- 
tive to  changes  in  the  condition  of  the  preparation. 
Changes  in  the  osmotic  pressure  of  the  surrounding 
fluid,  for  example,  have  a  strong  effect,  the  frequency 
increasing  reversibly  as  this  is  raised  (41,  44,  62).  A 
finding  which  is  important  in  indicating  a  possible  re- 
lation ijetween  electrical  events  in  the  nerve  and  these 
spontaneous  discharges  is  that  their  frequency  can 
be  altered  by  the  application  of  a  current  to  the 
nerve  which,  by  spreading  into  the  terminal  portion, 
will  alter  the  membrane  polarization  there  (23,  64). 
The  frequency  is  found  to  vary  approximately  ex- 
ponentially with  changes  in  the  polarizing  current  in 
the  nerve,  being  increased  by  depolarization  of  the 
terminals.  The  frequency  of  discharge  is  also  increased 
when  the  concentration  of  potassium  ions  in  the 
bathing  fluid  is  raised  above  the  normal  level,  this 
probably  operating  in  the  same  way  as  current  by- 
causing  a  reduction  of  membrane  potential. 

The  rate  of  rise  of  the  endplate  potential,  up  to  the 
level  at  which  an  action  potential  is  initiated,  is 
about  one  hundred  times  greater  than  the  mean  rate 
of  ri.se  of  the  miniature  endplate  potential.  A  decrease 
in  the  calcium  ion  concentration  of  the  solution  bath- 
ing the  preparation  causes  a  reduction  in  the  endplate 
potential,   while   the   amplitude   of  the  spontaneous 


208 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


discharge  is  left  unchanged;  the  same  effect  is  pro- 
duced by  the  addition  of  magnesium  ions  (4,  19, 
20,  21,  41,  63).  Calcium  appears  to  exert  a  specific 
facilitatory  action  on  the  release  of  acetylcholine  by  a 
nerv'e  impulse,  and  the  action  of  magnesium  may 
then  be  accounted  for  by  a  competition  with  calcium 
for  the  reactive  site.  This  antagonistic  relation  be- 
tween calcium  and  magnesium  at  the  terminals  is  in 
contrast  to  their  common  action  in  raising  the 
threshold  depolarization  for  the  initiation  of  an  action 
potential  in  a  nerve  or  muscle  fiber.  By  the  with- 
drawal of  calcium  or  addition  of  magnesium  or  both, 
the  endplate  potential  can  be  reduced  to  a  small 
fraction  of  its  normal  size  and  can  be  made  to  ap- 
proach in  amplitude  the  spontaneous  miniature  po- 
tential. When  this  is  done,  the  amplitude  of  the 
response  to  successive  nerve  impulses  is  seen  to  fluc- 
tuate widely,  in  contrast  to  its  constancy  under 
normal  conditions  or  when  the  response  is  reduced  to 
any  degree  by  treatment  with  curare.  With  the 
junction  sufficiently  deprived  of  calciimi,  the  response 
occurs  intermittently.  When  the  proportion  of  fail- 
ures is  large,  the  responses  to  a  series  of  ner\e  im- 
pulses have  a  distribution  of  amplitudes  similar  to 
that  of  the  spontaneous  discharge.  At  a  somewhat 
lower  level  of  depression,  the  distribution  shows 
several  peaks,  corresponding  to  small  integral  multi- 
ples of  the  mean  of  the  spontaneous  potentials.  It  is 
evident  that  the  endplate  potential  under  these  condi- 
tions is  composed  of  a  variable  whole  number  of 
miniature  endplate  potentials,  the  fluctuation  being 
due  to  variation  in  number  and  in  size  of  units.  The 
result  of  an  analysis  of  this  fluctuation  for  the  proba- 
bility of  occurrence  of  different  numbers  of  units  can 
be  accurately  fitted  by  a  Poisson  distribution.  This 
implies  that  there  is  no  interaction  between  units, 
the  probability  of  occurrence  of  each  being  unaffected 
by  the  number  of  units  composing  the  response. 

With  the  distribution  in  this  form,  relatively  large 
fluctuations  in  response  would  occur  only  when  the 
number  of  contributing  units  is  small.  As  the  number 
increases,  the  amplitude  of  fluctuation  relative  to  the 
mean  amplitude  of  response  will  vary  in  inverse 
proportion  to  the  square  root  of  the  number  of  units, 
while  the  additional  dispersion  due  to  variation  in 
amplitude  of  individual  units  will  become  pro- 
gressively less  significant.  Fluctuations  occur  in  the 
curarized  endplate  potential,  evoked  under  condi- 
tions in  which  the  release  of  acetylcholine  from  the 
nerve  terminals  is  normal,  and  these  can  be  attributed 
to  a  variation  in  the  number  of  units  around  such  a 
magnitude  as  would  be  predicted  roughly  from  the 


size  of  the  normal  response  (68).  The  probability 
that  the  normal  endplate  potential  is  composed  of 
these  units  is  greatly  strengthened  by  the  observa- 
tion that  an  increase  in  the  calcium  ion  concentra- 
tion beyond  that  normally  present  in  the  bathing 
fluid  produces  a  further  increase  in  the  size  of  the 
response,  which  is  entirely  attributable  to  an  increase 
in  the  release  of  acetylcholine  from  the  nerve  ter- 
minals, and  which  is  presumably  due  to  an  increase 
in  the  probability  of  release  of  individual  units  of 
acetylcholine  (13,  29,  43,  52).  The  curarized  end- 
plate potential  can  in  this  way  be  increased  two  or 
three  times  in  size. 

In  contrast  with  the  effect  on  the  response  to  a 
nerve  impulse,  changes  in  the  calcium  concentration 
in  cither  direction  from  normal  are  usually  found 
to  have  no  effect  on  the  frequency  of  spontaneous 
miniature  potentials.  Calcium  withdrawal  (or  mag- 
nesium addition)  does,  however,  reduce  the  fre- 
quency when  this  has  first  been  raised  by  the  presence 
of  a  high  concentration  of  potassium  ions  or  by  a 
current  applied  to  depolarize  the  nerve  terminals.  It 
thus  appears  that  the  depletion  of  calcium  ions  has  a 
similar  action  in  presenting  an  increase  in  the  proba- 
bility of  a  unit  of  acetylcholine  being  released  during 
a  given  time  interval  by  a  maintained  depolariza- 
tion, as  in  reducing  the  probability  of  its  release  by  a 
nerve  impulse. 

Another  procedure  which  modifies  the  number  of 
units  responding  to  a  nerve  impulse  is  the  previous 
activation  of  the  nerve.  In  the  curarized  amphibian 
muscle,  the  second  of  two  closely  spaced  nerve  im- 
pulses elicits  a  larger  endplate  potential  than  does  the 
first  (30,  43,  72).  With  continued  repetitive  stimula- 
tion of  the  nerve,  the  individual  responses  increase 
progressively  until  a  steady  condition  is  attained.  By 
this  means  the  size  of  the  response  may  be  increased 
to  two  or  three  times  that  elicited  by  an  isolated  im- 
pulse. (This  increase  is  in  addition  to  the  summation  of 
electrical  changes  in  the  postjunctional  structure,  the 
later  responses  adding  to  the  potential  change  remain- 
ing from  previous  responses.)  In  the  case  where  two  im- 
pulses are  set  up  in  the  nerve  the  potentiation  of  the 
response  to  the  second  is  found  to  be  greatest  at  the 
shortest  interval  of  time  at  which  the  ners-e  will  con- 
duct. The  effect  falls  gradually  as  the  interval  be- 
tween the  nerve  impulses  is  increased,  the  response 
having  returned  to  its  unpotentiated  size  at  an  inter- 
val of  about  100  msec.  That  this  potentiation  is  a 
prejunctional  phenomenon  and  moreover  that  it  in- 
volves a  change  in  the  number  of  units  of  acetylcho- 
line released  is  revealed  by  studying  the  effect  under 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


209 


conditions  in  which  the  number  of  units  responding 
to  a  nerve  impulse  is  small.  For  this  purpose  the  cal- 
cium concentration  is  reduced  (or  magnesium  added) 
until  the  response  to  a  single  nerve  impulse  has  a 
mean  amplitude  of  one  or  a  few  units.  With  two 
nerve  impulses  at  a  short  interval  apart  the  response 
to  the  second  is  found  to  be  statistically  larger,  as  in 
the  curarized  preparation.  Examination  of  the  distri- 
bution of  amplitudes  for  the  first  and  second  re- 
sponses in  a  number  of  trials  reveals  that  the  increase 
in  the  second  is  accompanied  by  a  reduction  in  its 
fluctuation,  indicating  that  the  change  is  entirely 
the  result  of  an  increase  in  the  number  of  units  re- 
sponding (22).  It  is  further  found  that  the  number  of 
units  responding  to  the  first  nerve  impulse  in  a  par- 
ticular trial  has  no  effect  on  the  number  responding 
to  the  second  in  that  trial.  This  leads  to  the  conclusion 
that  the  potentiation  of  the  second  response  depends 
solely  on  the  previous  presence  of  an  impulse  in  the 
nerve  and  not  on  the  number  of  units  of  acetylcholine 
released  by  the  impulse. 

Whereas  in  the  amphibian  the  second  of  two 
ner\e  impulses  elicits  an  endplate  potential  which  is 
larger  than  the  first,  in  the  curarized  mammalian 
preparation  the  response  to  the  second  is  smaller  up 
to  an  interval  of  a  few  seconds  (30,  65,  66).  Evidence 
of  potentiation  by  previous  activity  of  the  nerve  is 
procured  where  the  conditioning  treatment  is  a  large 
number  of  ner\e  impulses.  When  between  a  few 
hundred  and  a  few  thousand  impulses  are  set  up  in 
the  nerve  within  5  to  20  sec,  the  later  impulses  in  the 
train  elicit  a  considerably  reduced  response  owing  to 
the  depressant  efifect  of  preceding  volleys.  The  time 
course  of  subsequent  changes  in  the  effectiveness  of 
transmission  is  revealed  by  testing  with  a  single  im- 
pulse at  a  variable  time  after  the  termination  of  the 
conditioning  train  of  impulses.  It  is  thus  found  that 
the  effectiveness  of  transmission  gradually  increases 
from  the  depressed  state  to  beyond  that  occurring  in 
the  absence  of  previous  activity  (5,  48,  65).  The  mag- 
nitude and  time  course  of  this  potentiation  depends 
on  the  number  of  conditioning  nerve  impulses;  it  is 
larger,  arises  later  and  is  more  prolonged,  the  greater 
the  number  of  impulses.  Following  a  few  thousand 
impulses,  the  maximum  is  not  reached  until  about  0.5 
min.  after  conditioning,  when  the  response  as  meas- 
ured by  the  size  of  the  endplate  potential  may  be  50 
per  cent  greater  than  the  normal  and  the  total  dura- 
tion of  the  potentiated  state  may  be  i  o  min. 

When  the  curare-free  mammalian  preparation  is 
subjected  to  calciuin  depletion,  a  behavior  is  observed 
which  is  similar  to  that  in  the  frog.  The  second  of  two 


closely  spaced  nerve  impulses  now  elicits  a  greater 
response  than  the  first  (67).  The  effect  of  condition- 
ing with  a  train  of  impulses  is  to  cause  a  summation 
of  the  potentiation  left  behind  by  individual  nerve 
impulses.  It  is  apparent  that  the  potentiation  in  the 
wake  of  a  nerve  impulse  has  a  very  prolonged  phase 
of  low  level  effectiveness,  which,  while  hardly  notice- 
able after  a  single  impulse,  is  able  to  sum  over  a  large 
number  of  impulses  to  produce  an  appreciable  po- 
tentiation of  very  great  duration.  When  the  calcium 
concentration  is  normal,  the  earlier  part  of  this  po- 
tentiation is  outweighed  by  the  depression  which 
follows  each  nerve  impulse  but  does  not  sum  over  as 
long  a  period  of  time.  The  fact  that  the  depression 
does  not  occur  in  the  calcium  depleted  preparation 
when  the  number  of  units  of  acetylcholine  released  by 
each  impulse  is  small  makes  it  appear  highly  probable 
that  this  effect,  unlike  the  potentiation,  depends  on 
the  amount  of  acetylcholine  released  by  previous 
impulses. 

In  the  mammalian  muscle  under  normal  condi- 
tions, the  frequency  of  spontaneous  discharges  is 
found  to  be  increased  immediately  following  the 
response  to  a  conditioning  nerve  impulse  at  which 
time  the  response  to  a  second  impulse  is  diminished. 
After  conditioning  with  a  large  number  of  impulses, 
the  frequency  is  increased  many  times  and  returns 
only  very  slowly  to  normal  (6,  62).  The  final  part  of 
its  return  parallels  the  time  course  of  the  subsidence 
of  the  potentiation  of  transmission,  as  observed  in  the 
curarized  muscle.  The  effect  of  previous  activity  of  the 
nerve  is  apparently  to  increase  the  potentiality  of  the 
terminals  for  releasing  units  of  acetylcholine,  both 
spontaneously  and  in  response  to  a  nerve  impulse. 


PROPERTIES    OF    THE    JUNCTIONAL    RECEPTOR 

The  most  direct  method  for  investigating  the 
receptive  properties  of  the  muscle  fiber  is  to  add  acetyl- 
choline to  the  surrounding  fluid  without  involving 
the  nerve  terminals.  Two  techniques  have  been  used: 
the  acetylcholine  has  been  applied  either  uniformly 
to  the  whole  muscle  fiber,  or  in  a  highly  localized 
manner  to  the  region  contacted  by  the  nerve  endings. 
The  effect  is  a  depolarization  of  the  muscle  fiber  in 
the  junctional  region  (12,  17,  36,  51).  After  pre- 
liminary treatment  with  an  anticholinesterase,  which 
prevents  the  enzymatic  destruction  of  acetylcholine, 
the  technique  of  uniform  application  allows  quantita- 
tive information  to  be  obtained  on  the  reactivity  of 
the  receptor  with  varying  concentrations  of  acetyl- 


210 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


.NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


choline.  When  the  acetylcholine  concentration  is  as 
high  as  I  jumole  per  liter,  muscle  fibers  are  depolarized 
sufficiently  for  spikes  to  be  initiated.  For  low  concen- 
trations, not  exceeding  that  required  to  elicit  spikes, 
the  depolarization  is  nearly  proportional  to  the  acetyl- 
choline concentration.  With  high  concentrations  the 
depolarization  elicited  by  acetylcholine  can  be  meas- 
ured in  the  wake  of  an  initial  burst  of  spikes,  when 
the  muscle  fiber  in  the  region  of  the  junction  is  re- 
fractory to  the  initiation  of  further  spikes.  At  the 
lower  concentrations  the  depolarization  is  maintained 
for  many  minutes  while  the  acetylcholine  remains  in 
the  surrounding  fluid;  at  the  higher  concentrations  a 
perceptible  decline  is  observed  within  a  few  minutes, 
the  rate  of  decline  being  greater  the  higher  the  con- 
centration of  acetylcholine.  This  effect  is  apparently 
the  result  of  a  gradual  desensitization  of  the  receptor 
by  its  forming  a  different  and  less  readily  reversible 
combination  with  acetylcholine  than  that  which 
results  in  depolarization. 

More  accurate  information  on  the  spatial  distribu- 
tion of  the  receptor  and  the  time  course  of  its  reaction 
may  be  obtained  by  applying  brief  pulses  of  acetyl- 
choline with  a  micropipette  (25,  70).  It  is  found  that 
the  high  sensitivity  to  acetylcholine  does  not  extend 
beyond  very  limited  regions  in  the  neighborhood  of 
the  nerve  terminal  branches,  for  in  the  frog,  where  the 
terminals  are  spread  over  about  a  200  /n  length  of  fiber, 
it  is  necessary  to  position  the  micropipette  to  within 
10  or  20  M  in  order  to  obtain  a  high  sensitivity.  It  is 
further  observed  that  acetylcholine  exerts  its  power- 
ful action  only  when  applied  externally;  it  has  no 
specific  effect  when  released  within  the  muscle  fiber, 
even  though  the  pipette  is  situated  immediately  be- 
neath a  region  of  the  fiber  surface  that  is  found  to  be 
sensitive  to  external  application.  With  the  micro- 
pipette critically  placed  over  the  junction  so  as  to 
obtain  maximum  sensitivity,  the  depolarization 
evoked  by  a  brief  pulse  of  acetylcholine  rises  to  a  peak 
in  about  15  msec.  This  order  of  lime  would  no  doubt 
be  required  for  the  diffusion  of  acetylcholine  from  its 
point  of  release  to  the  receptor  some  microns  away. 

Among  agents  that  affect  neuromuscular  trans- 
mission, the  one  that  has  received  most  attention  is 
curare.  This  term  applies  to  a  group  of  related  sub- 
stances which  act  by  competing  with  acetylcholine 
for  the  receptor.  Combination  of  curare  with  the  re- 
ceptor does  not  itself  aflfect  the  electrical  properties  of 
the  membrane,  but  it  prevents  acetylcholine  combin- 
ing and  thereby  exerting  a  depolarizing  action,  .\mong 
the  common  inorganic  ions,  sodium  appears  to  have 
the  most  marked  effect  on  the  combination  of  acetvl- 


choline  with  the  receptor  (36,  42).  After  the  complete 
withdrawal  of  sodium  ions  from  the  bathing  solu- 
tion, the  application  of  acetylcholine  elicits  a  small 
depolarization,  which  is  augmented  considerably  by 
the  presence  of  only  a  small  concentration  of  sodium. 
This  effect  is  not  produced  by  the  addition  of  calcium 
or  potassium  ions.  It  is  inferred  to  be  due  to  a  change 
in  the  reaction  between  the  receptor  and  acetylcholine, 
rather  than  in  a  later  stage  of  the  process  leading  to 
depolarization,  from  the  fact  that  sodium  ions  also  in- 
crease the  ability  of  acetylcholine  to  compete  with 
curare  for  the  receptor.  A  facilitation  of  the  reaction 
between  the  receptor  and  acetylcholine  in  muscles  of 
the  frog  is  also  produced  by  the  addition  to  the 
bathing  medium  of  very  small  concentrations  of  epi- 
nephrine and  norepinephrine,  the  substances  released 
by  impulses  at  the  terminals  of  sympathetic  post- 
ganglionic nerve  fibers  (49}. 

The  anticholinesterases  are  a  group  of  substances 
that  affect  transmission  by  competitively  inhibiting 
the  enzyme  cholinesterase,  which  is  concentrated  in 
the  junctional  region  of  the  muscle  fiber  and  normally 
hydrolyzes  acetylcholine  soon  after  its  liberation  from 
the  nerve  terminals.  Unlike  the  reaction  between  the 
receptor  and  acetylcholine  or  curare,  which  must  be 
very  rapid  in  reaching  an  equilibrium,  that  between 
the  enzyme  and  a  reversible  anticholinesterase  takes 
many  minutes.  With  the  anticholinesterase  exerting 
its  maximum  effect  and  presumably  completely  in- 
hibiting the  enzyme,  the  time  course  of  transmitter 
action  is  in  two  stages  (31,  40).  The  2  msec,  phase  of 
high  intensity  transmitter  action  is  virtually  un- 
changed and  accounts  for  the  early  rapid  rise  of  the 
endplate  potential.  This  is  succeeded  by  a  prolonged 
phase  of  low  level  transmitter  action  which  heightens 
and  prolongs  the  endplate  potential. 

Other  organic  compounds  besides  acetylcholine 
exert  a  depolarizing  action  at  the  junction.  Some  of 
the  substances  that  have  been  examined  combine  in 
various  degrees  the  properties  of  acetylcholine,  curare 
and  anticholinesterases  (32,  74).  In  the  case  where 
the  first  two  actions  are  combined,  the  agent  in  a 
concentration  which  produces  a  small  depolariza- 
tion prevents  acetylcholine  from  adding  to  this  to  the 
extent  obtaining  when  the  former  is  absent.  Different 
substances  are  found  to  follow  various  time  courses 
in  their  action,  and  where  the  same  one  exerts  multi- 
ple types  of  action,  each  may  develop  along  a  different 
time  course.  Furthermore  the  relative  effectiveness  for 
each  type  of  action  may  vary  between  different 
preparations. 

Transmission  would  be  expected  to  be  influenced  at 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


various  stages  by  changes  of  temperature.  The  most 
conspicuous  result  of  lowering  it  is  a  prolongation  of 
the  phase  of  transmitter  action.  This  appears  to  be 
due  largely  to  a  reduction  in  the  activit)  of  cholin- 
esterase  since  at  low  temperatures  treatment  with  an 
anticholinesterase  produces  little  additional  change 
(4,  31).  It  is  found,  however,  that,  while  the  time 
course  of  the  curarized  endplate  potential  is  length- 
ened, the  peak  amplitude  is  not  significantly  in- 
creased as  it  should  be  if  the  early  phase  of  transmitter 
action  were  unaltered.  In  the  mammalian  muscle  this 
appears  to  be  the  result  of  curare  competing  more 
effectively  with  acetylcholine  at  the  reduced  tempera- 
ture and  thus  ofTsetting  the  effect  of  the  reduction  in 
cholinesterase  activity  on  the  peak  potential  change. 
An  experiment,  highly  relevant  to  the  conclusion 
that  the  alteration  of  the  properties  of  the  muscle 
fiber  produced  by  a  nerve  impulse  is  consistent  with 
the  operation  of  a  chemical  mediator,  is  the  demon- 
stration that  the  depolarization  elicited  by  acetyl- 
choline has  its  origin  in  the  same  conductance  change 
that  has  been  shown  to  occur  during  transmission 
(26).  For  this  purpose  the  muscle  has  first  been 
nearly  completely  depolarized  by  immersing  it  in  a 
solution  with  a  high  concentration  of  potassium  ions. 
In  this  condition  the  application  of  acetylcholine  pro- 
duces no  discernible  change  in  inembrane  potential. 
When  the  membrane  is  now  polarized  in  either  direc- 
tion by  the  passage  of  current  across  it,  acetylcholine 
produces  a  potential  change  that  partly  compensates 
for  the  displacement  from  the  unpolarized  state,  and 
this  is  attributable  to  an  increase  in  membrane  con- 
ductance similar  to  that  observed  for  the  preparation 
initially  in  its  normal  environment. 


CONCLUSION  :    MECHANISM    OF    TR.ANSMISSION 

From  the  rate  at  which  acetylcholine  appears  in  the 
effluent  from  a  perfused  muscle  during  repetitive 
stimulation  of  the  motor  nerve  fibers,  it  has  been  es- 
timated that  the  quantity  released  from  the  nerve 
endings  at  a  single  junction  in  response  to  a  single 
nerve  impulse  is  about  io~'-  moles  (i,  35).  Although 
the  value  obtained  in  this  way  is  liable  to  be  too  small 
because  of  losses  in  the  collection  procedure  and  be- 
cause of  a  depression  in  the  release  mechanism  by 
previous  activity,  it  is  not  likely  to  be  in  error  in  its 
order  of  magnitude.  It  may  be  compared  with  the 
minimum  quantity  of  about  5  X  lo""'  moles  of  acetyl- 
choline which  is  required  to  evoke  a  muscle  action 
potential  when  applied  to  the  junctional  region  by  a 


micropipette  (25,  70).  The  factor  of  200  between  these 
two  quantities  can  be  satisfactorily  accounted  for  by 
the  geometry  of  the  junction.  The  nerve  endings  from 
which  the  acetylcholine  is  released  are  probably  every- 
where in  very  close  proximity  to  the  receptive  region 
of  the  postjunctional  surface  with  a  consequent  high 
efficiency  for  its  reaching  the  receptor.  On  the  other 
hand,  when  acetylcholine  is  applied  by  a  micro- 
pipette, it  would  have  to  diffuse  over  a  greater  distance 
and  be  considerably  dispersed  before  reacting  with 
the  receptor,  and  a  larger  quantity'  would  therefore 
be  required  to  produce  a  comparable  effect.  Even  if 
the  micropipette  were  placed  directly  on  a  sensitive 
region,  the  application  of  a  moderate  amount  of 
acetylcholine  would  no  doubt  lead  to  a  rapid  satura- 
tion and  inactivation  of  the  receptor  there  because  of 
its  high  local  concentration,  and  the  initiation  of  an 
action  potential  would  require  the  action  of  acetyl- 
choline over  a  greater  part  of  the  receptive  area. 

From  the  concentration  of  acetylcholine  required  to 
produce  an  action  potential  when  applied  uniformly 
to  the  preparation  and  from  the  quantity  that  is  re- 
leased by  a  nerve  impulse,  it  is  possible  to  calculate 
the  volume  in  which  the  acetylcholine  released  from 
the  nerve  terminals  would  be  present  when  reacting 
with  the  receptor  (37).  The  result  shows  that  the 
acetylcholine  must  exert  its  maximum  effect  before 
diffusing  more  than  i  //,  a  distance  which  is  consistent 
with  morphological  findings  on  the  minute  separation 
of  the  pre-  and  postjunctional  surfaces.  Furthermore, 
assuming  that  diffusion  occurs  away  from  the  im- 
mediate neighborhood  of  the  junction,  the  time  dur- 
ing which  the  acetylcholine  will  remain  in  an  effective 
concentration  is  shown  to  be  less  than  i  msec.  The 
brief  duration  of  transmitter  action  may  reflect  the 
operation  of  this  diffusion,  though  the  possibility  re- 
mains that  the  reaction  between  the  receptor  and 
acetylcholine  does  not  reach  an  equilibrium  in  such  a 
short  period  of  time  and  the  kinetics  of  this  reaction 
may  then  influence  the  time  course  of  transmitter 
action.  At  least  it  is  clear  that  the  enzymatic  destruc- 
tion of  acetylcholine  is  not  involved  in  the  early,  high 
intensity  phase  of  transmitter  action,  as  it  is  not 
affected  by  the  presence  of  an  anticholinesterase.  The 
failure  of  the  destruction  of  acetylcholine  adds  a 
later  low  level  phase  of  transmitter  action  which 
probably  occurs  after  the  acetylcholine  has  diffused 
away  from  the  immediate  neighborhood  of  the 
terminals  where  it  is  released  and  is  dispersed  over 
the  entire  junctional  region. 

The  high  degree  of  chemical  specificity  of  the 
receptor  and  the  competition  for  it  of  different  sub- 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Stances  with  different  final  effects  is  suggestive  of  the 
behavior  of  an  enzyme.  It  is  highly  relevant  to  this 
that  substances  which  are  able  to  displace  acetyl- 
choline from  the  enzyme  cholinesterase  are  also  able 
to  displace  it  from  the  receptor.  The  receptor  appears 
almost  certainly  to  be  a  protein  constituent  of  the 
muscle  fiber  membrane  with  its  reactive  sites  exposed 
on  the  outer  surface.  As  a  result  of  the  combination 
of  these  sites  with  acetylcholine,  the  physical  proper- 
ties of  the  membrane  alter  and  a  new  path  appears 
for  the  diffusion  of  ions  of  various  species  through  it. 
In  electrical  terms  transmitter  action  may  be  ap- 
proximately described  as  the  placing  of  an  addi- 
tional conductance  across  the  membrane  which  short- 
circuits  any  previously  existing  potential  difference. 
In  that  the  experimental  findings  are  in  agreement 
with  this  interpretation,  they  exclude  the  possibility 
of  electrical  transmission  by  which  the  junctional 
response  is  considered  to  be  produced  by  an  externally 


generated  current  impressed  upon  the  muscle  fiber. 
At  the  same  time  they  eliminate  the  possibility  that 
the  response  may  be  of  the  nature  of  a  local  response, 
a  specific  increase  in  membrane  permeability  to 
sodium  ions  boosting  an  initially  small  potential 
change,  such  as  may  occur  when  the  membrane  is 
depolarized  to  near  the  threshold  for  setting  up  an 
action  potential.  It  appears  that  the  junctional 
respon.se  cannot  be  brought  about  by  any  means  of 
electrical  stimulation  of  the  postjunctional  structure 
but  only  by  a  specific  chemical  reaction  of  the  re- 
ceptor. The  presence  at  the  junction  of  a  region  capa- 
ble of  responding  in  this  way  does  not  appear  to 
affect  the  action  potential  developed  there,  except 
by  an  addition  of  the  independent  actions  of  the  two 
types  of  activity.  The  probable  significance  of  this  is 
that  the  area  occupied  by  the  receptor  is  small  and 
does  not  detract  appreciably  from  the  area  engaged 
in  producing  the  action  potential. 


REFERENCES 


12. 

13- 

14. 

'5- 
16. 

17- 
18. 


20. 
21. 

22. 

24. 

25- 
26. 
27. 
28. 

•^9- 


AcHESON,  G.  H.  Fed.  Proc.  7:  447,  1948. 

Bernard,    C.    Lemons  sur   les   effels   de   substances   toxiques   et 

medicamenteuses.  Paris:  Bailliere,  1857,  p.  267. 

Bov'D,  I.  A.  AND  A.  R.  Martin.  J.  Physiol.  132:  61,  1956. 

Boyd,  I.  A.  and  A.  R.  Martin.  J.  Physiol.  132:  74,  1956. 

Boyd,  T.  E.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  100:  569,  1932. 

Brooks,  V.  B.  J.  Physiol.  1 34 :  264,  1 956. 

Brown,  G.  L.  J.  Physiol.  89:  220,  1937. 

Brown,  G.  L.  J.  Physiol.  89:  438,  1937. 

Brown,  G.  L.,  H.  H.  Dale  and  W.  Feldberg.  J.  Phynoi 

87:  394.  1936. 

Burke,  W.  and  B.  L.  Ginsborg.  J.  Physiol.  132:  586,  1956. 
Burke,  W.,  and  B.  L.  Ginsborg.  J.  Physiol.  132:  599,  1956. 
Burns,  B.  D.  and  W.  D.  M.  Paton.  J.  Physiol.  115:41,  1 95 1 . 
Coppee,  G.  Arch,  internal,  physiol.  53:  327,  1943. 
CouTE-\ux,  R.  Pev.  Canad.  Biol.  6:  563,  1947. 
CouTEAUX,  R.  Internal.  Rev.  Cytol.  4:  335,  1955. 
CouTEAUX,   R.   and  J.   Taxi.   Arch.   Microsc.   Morphol.   41  : 

352,  1952. 

Cowan,  S.  L.  J.  Physwl.  88:  3P,   1936. 

Dale,  H.  H.,  W.  Feldberg  and  M.  Vogt.  J.  Physwl.  86: 

353.  '936. 


del  Castillo, 

'954- 

DEL  Castillo, 
del  Castillo, 
del  Castillo, 
del  c.\stillo, 
del  Castillo, 
DEL  Castillo, 
DEL  Castillo, 

DEL  CiVSTILLO, 

DEL  Castillo, 
Chem.  6:  121,  i 
del  Castillo 


J.   and   L.    Engbaek.  J.   Physiol.    124:   370, 


J.  AND  B.  Katz.  J.  Physwl.  124:  553,  1954 

J.  AND  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  124:  560,  1954 

J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physwl.  124:  574,  1954. 

J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  124:  586,  1954 

J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  125:  546,  1954 

J.  AND  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  128:  157,  1955 

J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  128:  396,  1955 

J.  AND  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  132:  630,  1956 
J.  AND  B.  Katz.  Prog.   Biophys.  &  Biophys 

956- 

J.  AND  L.  Stark.  J.  Physiol,   i  16:  507,  1952. 


30.  EccLES,  J.   C,   B.   Katz   and  .S.   W.   Kuffler.  J.  .Neuro- 
physiol.  4:  362,  1 941. 

31.  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.   Katz  and  S.  VV.  Kuffler.  J.  .Nemo- 
physiol.  5:  211,  1942. 

32.  EccLES,  J.   C.   AND  \V.   V.   MacFarlane.  J.  .\'europhyitol. 

12:  59.  1949- 

33.  Eccles,  J.  C.  AND  VV.  J   O'Connor.  J.  PM™/.  94:  9?,  1938. 

34.  Edmunds,  C.  \V.  and  G.  B.  Roth.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  23 :  28, 
1908. 

35.  Emmelin,  N.  and  F.  C.  Macintosh.  J.  Physiol.   131     477, 
1956. 

36.  Fatt,  P.   7-  Physiol.  111:  408,  1 950. 

37.  Fatt,  P.  Physiol.  Rev.  34;  674,  1954. 

38.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  1 1 1  :  46P,  1950. 

39.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  Nature,  London  166:  597,  1950. 

40.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  115:  320,  1951. 

41.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  117:  109,  1952. 

42.  Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  118:  73,  1952. 

43.  Feng,  T.  P.  Chinese  J.  Physiol.  15:  367,  1940. 

44.  Furshpan,  E.J.  J.  Physwl.  134:  689,  1956. 

45.  Gopert,   H.    and   H.    Schaefer.    Arch.   ges.    Physiol.    239: 

597.  1938. 

46.  Heidenhain,  R.  .irch.  .-inat.  Physiol.,  Physiol,  .ibt.  Suppl.,  7: 
■  33,  1883. 

47.  Hunt,  C.  C.  and  S.  \V.  Kuffler.  J.  Physiol.  126:  293,  1954. 

48.  Hutter,  O.  F.  J.  Physwl.  118;  216,  1952. 

49.  Hutter,  O.  F.  .\nd  VV.  R.  Loewenstein.  J.  Physiol.   130: 

559.  1955- 

50.  Katz,  B.  and  S.  VV.  Kuffler.  J.  ,\europhyswl.  4:  209,  1941. 

51.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  .Veurophysiol .  6:  99,  1943. 

52.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  7:17,  1944. 

53.  Kuffler,  S.  VV.  and  R.  W.  Gerard.  J.  Neurophysiol.  10: 

383.  1947- 

54.  Kuffler,    S.    VV.    and    E.    M.    Vaughan    Williams.    J. 
Physiol.   121  :  289,  1953. 


SKELETAL    NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION 


213 


55.  KUFFLER,    S.    W.    AND    E.    M.    Vaughan    VVilliams.    J. 
Physiol.   121:  318,   1953. 

56.  KiJHNE,  W.  ^ischr.  Biol.  23:  i,  1887. 

57.  Langley,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  33:  374,  1905. 

58.  Langlev,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  36:  347,  1907. 

59.  Langley,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  37:  285,  1908. 

60.  Langley,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  39:  235,  1909. 

61.  Langley,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  48:  73,  1914. 

62.  Liley,  a.  W.  jf.  Physiol.  132:  650,  1956. 

63.  Liley,  A.  W.  J.  Physiol.  133:  571,  1956. 

64.  Liley,  A.  W.  J.  Physiol.  134:  427,  1956. 

65.  Liley,  A.  VV.  and  K.  A.   K.  North    J.  .Neurophysiol.    16: 
509.  '953- 


66.  LuNDBERG,    A.    AND    H.    QuiLisCH.    Acta  physiol.    scandwai'. 
Suppl.  MI,  30:  III,  1953. 

67.  LuNDBERG,    A.    AND   H.    QuiLiscH.    Ada  physioi.   scandtnav. 
Suppl.  Ill,  30:  121,  1953. 

68.  Martin,  A.  R.  J.  Physiol.  130:  114,  1955. 

69.  Nastuk,  W.  L.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  42 :  249,  1953. 

70.  Nastuk,  VV.  L.  Fed.  Proc.  12:  102,  1953. 

7 1 .  Robertson,  J.  D.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem.  Cylol.  2  :  381 ,  1 956. 

72.  Schaefer,  H.  and  p.  Haass.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  242:  364, 

■939- 

73.  TiEGS,  O.  W.  Physiol.  Rev.  33:  90,  1953. 

74.  Zaimis,  E.J.  J.  Physiol.  122:  238,  1953. 


CHAPTER     VII 


Autonomic  neuroefFector  transmission 


U.   S.    V  O  N   E  U  L  E  R     I     Department  of  Physiology,  Faculty  of  Medicine,  Stockholm,  Sweden 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Development  of  the  Concept 
Anatomical  Considerations 
Humoral  Versus  Electrical  Transmission 
T'^e  Adrenergic  Nerve  Transmitter 
dentification 
Occurrence,  Biosynthesis  and  Storage  of  Adrenergic  Nerve 

Transmitter 
Release 

Influence  of  stimulation  frequency 
Effects  on  remote  organs 
Stimulation  of  isolated  nerves 
Exhaustibility 
Removal  of  Transmitter 
Possible  Adrenergic  Nerve  Transmitters  Other  Than  Norepi- 
nephrine 
The  Cholinergic  Nerve  Transmitter 
Identification 

Occurrence,  Biosynthesis  and  Storage 
Release  in  Organs 

Release  from  isolated  nerves 
Removal  of  Transmitter 
Mechanism  of  Action  of  Neurotransmitters 
Neurotransmitters  in  Blood  and  Urine 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  CONCEPT 

THE  IDEA  OF  CHEMICAL  TRANSMISSION  of  nerve  im- 
pulses was  apparently  first  expressed  by  Elliott  (41) 
who  in  1904  suggested  the  possibility  that  when  the 
sympathetic  nerve  impulse  reached  the  target  cell  it 
caused  an  action  by  liberating  epinephrine  "on  each 
occasion  when  the  impulse  arrives  at  the  periphery." 
This  hypothesis  was  based  on  the  similarities  in  action 
of  epinephrine  and  sympathetic  nerve  activity, 
irrespective  of  whether  the  action  was  activation  or 
inhibition. 

Elliott's  idea,  although  representing  an  entirely 
new  concept,  must  have  struck  many  as  plausible,  and 
it  was  not  surprising  that   thinking  should  proceed 


along  similar  lines.  Thus  Dixon  &  Hamill  (36)  ap- 
plied the  idea  to  parasyinpathetic  nerves,  comparing 
their  action  with  that  of  muscarine,  and  after  this 
time  it  became  primarily  a  matter  of  skillful  experi- 
mentation to  prove  the  correctness  of  the  theory  and 
to  carry  the  new  concept  to  general  acceptance.  This 
task  proved  more  difficult  than  was  perhaps  antici- 
pated. It  was  chiefly  due  to  the  precision  of  observa- 
tion and  judgment  of  Dale  (25)  and  the  ingenious 
experimentation  of  Loewi  (83)  that  the  postulate  of 
chemical  transmission  became  tran.sformed  into  an 
accepted  concept.  Acetylcholine  gradually  moved  into 
the  center  of  interest  as  a  possible  candidate  for 
parasympathetic  nerve  transmission.  In  Dale's  paper 
concerning  the  action  of  injected  acetylcholine,  he 
stated  that  it  caused  "pronounced  vagus-like  inhibi- 
tion of  the  heart,  and  various  other  effects  of  stimu- 
lating nerves  of  the  cranial  and  sacral  divisions  of  the 
autonomic  system — secretion  of  saliva,  contraction  of 
the  oesophagus,  stomach  and  intestine  and  of  the 
urinary  bladder." 

The  direct  experimental  proof  was  provided  by 
Loewi  (83)  who  showed  that  the  fluid  collected  from 
an  isolated  frog's  heart  during  vagus  stiinulation 
inhibited  a  second  heart  (fig.  i).  The  effect  of  the 
"Vagusstoff"  was  annulled  by  atropine  and  in  a  large 
series  of  experiments  it  could  be  shown  that  the 
liberated  substance  behaved  in  every  respect,  phar- 
macologically and  chemically,  like  a  choline  ester.  It 
is  generally  assumed  that  it  is  acetylcholine. 

Stimulation  of  the  sympathetic  nerves  in  Loewi's 
experiments  caused  the  release  of  a  factor  which 
accelerated  the  heart  and  had  properties  similar  to 
those  of  epinephrine.  Chemical  transmission  of 
sympathetic  nerve  impulses  was  independently  dem- 
onstrated by  Cannon  &  Uridil  (21)  who  found  that 
the  stimulation  of  hepatic  nerves  increased  the  rate 
of  the  denervated  heart  and  rai^d  the  arterial  pres- 


215 


2l6  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


liliililLMi 


.UiMJviiiUi 


FIG.  I.  Bain's  modification  of  the  original  experiment  performed  by  Loevvi  in  1921.  The  diagram 
represents  a  reservoir  of  salt  solution  from  which  there  is  a  passage  to  the  donor  heart  (D);  pressure 
trom  the  reservoir  assures  a  continuous  flow  of  the  solution  through  that  heart  to  the  recipient  heart 
(R).  The  donor  heart  still  has  its  proper  nerves.  Each  heart  is  attached  to  a  writing  lever.  The  record 
is  that  of  the  two  hearts,  donor  and  recipient.  When  the  vagal  fibers  of  the  donor  were  stimulated  (S), 
there  was  a  prompt  arrest  of  that  heart  (D),  and  later  a  slowing  and  arrest  of  the  recipient  heart 
(R),  with  gradual  recovery.  Time  (T)  is  recorded  in  5-sec.  intervals.  [From  Bain  (7).] 


FIG.  2.  Rise  of  arterial  pressure  and  increase  of  heart  rate  from 
196  to  220  beats  per  min.  following  stimulation  of  the  hepatic 
nerves  in  the  cat.  Time,  5  sec.  [From  Cannon  &  Uridil  (21).] 


sure  (fig.  2).  It  did  not  dilate  the  pupil,  howexer, 
which  would  have  been  expected  if  the  substance 
carried  by  the  blood  were  epinephrine. 

The  principle  of  chemical  transmission  was  later 
greatly  developed  chiefly  by  the  work  of  Cannon  & 
Rosenblueth  and  their  associates,  and  by  Dale, 
Feldberg,  Minz  and  their  co-workers.  A  very  useful 


distinction  was  introduced  by  Dale  (27)  when  the 
tcrins  adrenergic  and  cholinergic  nerves  were  coined 
(fig.  3).  While  acetylcholine  still  holds  the  position 
allotted  to  it  since  191 4  as  the  cholinergic  chemotrans- 
mitter,  the  concept  of  epinephrine  as  adrenergic 
transmitter  has  had  to  yield  to  its  nonmethylated 
homolosiue  norepinephrine  (124).  The  "curiously 
anomalous"  effect  on  the  iris  observed  by  Cannon  & 
Uridil  in  1921  (21)  i:)ecame  readilv  explained  by  the 
recognition  that  norepinephrine  and  not  epinephrine 
was  the  mediator  of  adrenergic  nerve  action. 

For  a  detailed  account  of  the  problem  of  autonomic 
neuroeffector  transmission  the  reader  is  referred  to  the 
monographs  of  Gaddum  (50),  Cannon  &  Ro.scn- 
blueth  (20),  Muralt  (133),  Rosenblueth  (113) 
Minz  (96,  97),  Euler  (129)  and  the  recent  survey  of 
neurochemistry  (loi). 


.^N.ATOMIC.^L    CONSIDER.ATIONS 

As  in  other  tields  of  ph\siolog"y,  valuable  hints  may 
be  gained  by  studying  the  microarchitecture  of  the 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


217 


region  in  question,  in  this  case  the  structural  relation- 
ships between  the  autonomic  postganglionic  nerve 
endings  and  the  target  cells.  These  cells  in  principle 
include  the  heart  muscle  cells  and  the  secretory  cells 
of  the  glands  in  addition  to  those  of  smooth  muscle. 
Much  conflicting  evidence  has  been  presented  with 
regard  to  the  innervation  of  smooth  muscle  cells  by 
autonomic  ner\e  fibers.  It  inay  sufhce  to  mention  that 
an  histologist  as  experienced  as  Slohr  (121)  found 
that  less  than  one  cell  in  a  hundred  was  inner\ated. 
The  numerous  reports  on  intracellular  nerve  endings 
in  smooth  muscle  cells  seem  to  require  reconsideration 
since  an  ingrowth  of  axonal  endings  into  a  cell  ap- 
pears for  many  reasons  unlikely,  and  even  unneces- 
sary, especially  in  view  of  the  probable  distribution  of 
the  transmitter  in  the  terminal  parts  of  the  axons, 
to  be  discussed  later.  It  must  therefore  be  seriously 
considered  whether  the  alleged  findings  are  not  due  to 
misinterpretation  of  the  histological  pictures.  It  is 
well  known  that  smooth  muscle  cells  may  .serve  their 
proper  function  without  innervation,  and  unless  it 
can  be  shown  that  each  smooth  muscle  cell  receives 
intracellular  nerve  twigs  there  is  every  reason  to 
regard  the  few  exceptions  known  at  present  as  interest- 
ing special  cases  of  unknown  functional  significance. 
The  finding  of  numerous  endings  in  the  ciliary  muscle 
of  the  eye  does  not  alter  the  general  picture.  There  is 
nothing  known  so  far  to  indicate  any  kind  of  motor 
end  plate'  on  the  sinooth  muscle  cell.  Knoblike 
thickenings  ending  at  or  near  the  cell  surface  have 
been  described,  however,  both  by  older  histologists 
and  more  recently.  Similar  structures,  sometimes 
assuming  the  picture  of  bead-strings,  have  been  re- 
peatedly found  at  autonomic  nerve  endings  (54,  62, 
72).  Garven  &  Gairns  suggest  "that  the  small  beads 
on  the  course  of  the  finest  fibrils  represent  the  actual 
release  points  of  the  humoral  agents  within  the  cyto- 
plasmic continuum  provided  by  cells  other  than  the 
neurones.  " 

As  will  be  discussed  in  the  following  section  the 
results  of  studies  of  electrical  phenoinena  in  the 
siTiooth  muscles  do  not  suggest  direct  innervation  of 
such  cells. 

Cannon  &  Rosenblueth  (20)  have  regarded  the 
few  innervated  cells  as  having  special  functions  and 
have  named  them  'key  cells.'  Their  contention  was 
that  by  chemical  transmission  concentrated  to  these, 
the  neighboring  cells  will  be  affected  by  the  diflfusing 
neurotransmitter.  There  is  little  evidence  to  support 
this  hypothesis,  however.  Moreover,  since  it  is  known 
that   the   autonomic   nerve   transmitters   are   presept 


FIG.  3.  Dale's  schpmatic  representation  of  the  autonomic 
nervous  system.  A,  adrenergic;  C,  cholinergic  elements. 
[From  Dale  (28).] 

all  along  the  axons,  it  is  unlikely  that  they  should 
be  released  only  at  one  point  of  the  axon  in  a  small 
nuinber  of  special  cells. 

The  question  of  the  innervation  of  the  smooth  mus- 
cle cell  cannot  be  answered  with  coinplete  certainty 
but  the  best  evidence  points  at  a  peripheral  branching 
system  of  the  postganglionic  autonomic  nerve  fibers 
extending  to  the  inynediate  neighborhood  of  each 
snipoth  muscle  cell  (62).  By  release  of  the  chemical 
transmitter  during  nerve  stimulation,  the  cells  will 
be  reached  by  the  active  chemical  substance  through 
diffusion.  The  proportion  of  cells  activated  in  an  organ 
and  the  degree  of  activation  will  clearly  depend  upon 
the  amount  of  transmitter  set  free,  which  in  its  turn 
is  a  function  of  the  frequency  and  strength  of  the 
stimulus  applied  to  the  nerve. 


HUMORAL   VERSUS   ELECTRICAL   TRANSMISSION 

The  bulk  of  evidence  points  to  the  conclusion  that 
denervated  smooth  muscle  is  electrically  inexcitable 
(100,  114).  Even  if  direct  stimulation  of  denervated 
smooth  iTiuscle  may  lead  to  contraction,  this  is  weak 
and  differs  in  several  respects  from  that  produced  by 
the  chemical  stimuli.  It  appears  likely  that  the  direct 
stimulation  effect  is  unspecific  and  due  to  a  direct 
gross  action  on  the  contractile  material.  An  important 


2l8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


argument  is  further  that  single  stimuli  are  not  capable 
of  eliciting  contractions.  Whether  the  negative  results 
of  stimulating  the  denervated  adrenal  medulla  (114) 
can  be  used  as  support  for  the  thesis  of  inexcitability  of 
denervated  target  cells  is  open  to  doubt. 

The  inexcitability  of  autonomic  effectors  has  also 
been  studied  by  'chemical  denervation,'  by  use  of 
drugs  which  block  the  action  of  the  autonomic  nerves 
on  the  target  cells.  Such  experiments  have  been  made 
on  the  piloerectors  after  ergotoxin  (20)  and  on  the 
salivary  gland  cells  after  chlorpromazine  (42). 

It  may  therefore  be  concluded  that  the  smooth  mus- 
cle cell  lacks  the  ability  to  respond  to  direct  electrical 
stimulation.  Since  there  is  ample  evidence  to  show 
that  these  cells  respond  readily  to  the  chemical 
stimuli  which  are  known  to  Ije  released  from  the 
terminal  parts  of  the  autonomic  postganglionic  nerves, 
there  .seems  to  be  no  need  to  postulate  electrical 
transmission  for  functional  reasons. 

For  a  detailed  discussion  of  the  dual  theory  of 
chemical  and  electrical  transmission  advocated  by 
Monnier  &  Bacq  (100)  see  Cannon  &  Rosenblueth 
(20}.  While  there  is  no  evidence  for  electrical  trans- 
mission from  the  postganglionic  autonomic  nerve 
fiber  to  the  effector  cell,  the  situation  may  be  different 
in  the  case  of  autonomic  synapses  (loi). 

Smooth  muscle  thus  differs  fundamentally  from 
skeletal  mu.scle  in  that  the  latter  is  rapidly  activated 
by  a  trigger  mechanism  requiring  direct  contact  i:)e- 
tween  the  nerve  fiber  and  the  effector  and  working  on 
the  all-or-none  principle.  The  sustained  activity  of 
the  smooth  muscle  appears  to  operate  on  the  entirely 
different  principle  of  graded  responses  (115).  More 
data  are  required,  however,  before  the  activity  of  the 
single  smooth  muscle  cell  in  response  to  physiological 
stimuli  can  be  ascertained. 


by  biological  tests  and  by  colorimetric  methods  (1^9)- 
For  the  identification  of  the  transmitter  the  differ- 
entiation from  epinephrine  became  of  primary  im- 
portance. On  most  target  cells  the  actions  of  epineph- 
rine and  norepinephrine  are  qualitatively  similar, 
but  the  relative  activity  varies  from  one  organ  to 
another.  Thus  the  action  of  epinephrine  may  be 
over  one  hundred  times  that  of  norepinephrine  on  the 
rat's  uterus  and  on  the  fowl's  rectal  cecum  while  the 
two  amines  ha\e  about  the  same  activity  on  the  iso- 
lated heart.  By  comparing  the  actions  of  the  purified 
extracts  containing  the  neurotransmitter  on  a  series 
of  test  preparations  it  is  possible  to  ascertain  whether 
the  relative  actions  of  the  unknown  compound  go 
parallel  with  one  or  the  other  of  the  standard  sub- 
stances. Though  norepinephrine  passed  unnoticed 
by  chemical  tests  in  the  so-called  pure  crystalline 
epinephrine  prepared  from  suprarenals  for  nearly 
50  years,  the  amines  are  now  readily  separated  by 
chromatography  (73). 

Generally  a  single  pair  of  test  objects  showing 
sufficiently  large  differences  in  the  activity'  ratio  be- 
tween epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  suffice  for 
differentiation  between  the  two  amines.  Suitable 
pairs  are  for  instance  the  cat's  arterial  pressure  and 
the  fowl's  rectal  cecum.  On  the  former  preparation 
norepinephrine  is  from  i  to  5  times  more  active  as  a 
pressor  agent  than  epinephrine,  while  it  has  only  '  4 
to  J200  of  the  activity  of  epinephrine  on  the  fowl's 
rectal  cecum  (fig.  4). 

The  virgin  uterus  of  the  cat,  and  the  iris  are  5  to  10 
times  more  sensitive  to  epinephrine  than  to  norepi- 
nephrine and  may  be  u.sed  for  differentiating  pur- 
poses. The  rat's  uterus  under  certain  conditions  is 
stimulated  by  norepinephrine  and  relaxed  by  epi- 
nephrine (fig.  5). 


THE    ADRENERGIC    NERVE    TRANSMITTER 

Identification 

As  outlined  in  the  introductory  section,  Loewi's 
experiments  in  1921  supported  the  idea  that  the 
sympathetic  (adrenergic)  transmitter  was  epineph- 
rine-like.  The  suggestions  by  Barger  &  Dale  (9),  Bacq 
(4)  and  Greer,  Pinkston,  Baxter  &  Brannon  (58)  that 
norepinephrine  conformed  better  with  the  actions  of 
the  sympathetic  transmitter  than  did  epinephrine  re- 
ceived little  attention  until  it  was  shown  by  von  Euler 
(124)  that  the  adrenergic  nerves  contained  not  epi- 
nephrine but  norepinephrine.  The  identification  of 
the  transmitter  with  le\'o-norepinephrine  was  proved 


FIG.  4.  Effect  of  epinepfirine  (/-adr),  norepinephrine  (/- 
nor-adr)  and  extract  of  beef  splenic  nerves  (Spl.  n.)  on  the 
arterial  pressure  of  the  cat  and  on  the  isolated  rectal  cecum  of 

the  fowl.  [From  von  Euler  (128).] 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


219 


0.2    ng  0.2    (IF,  0.1    ug      0.2    |ig 

Koradr.  Adr.  Ijore.dr.     Adr. 

FIG.  5.  Rat  uterus,  3  hours  post  partum.  o.i  and  0.2  Mg 
norepinephrine  stimulates,  0.2  Mg  epinephrine  inhibits  the 
uterus.  [From  GreetT  &  Hokz  (56).] 


It  has  been  observed  for  some  time  that  although 
the  actions  of  epinephrine  on  the  arterial  pressure  of 
the  cat  may  be  reversed  by  antisympathomimetic 
substances  (ergotoxine,  yohimbine,  benzodioxane, 
dibenamine,  phentolamine),  the  effects  of  sympa- 
thetic nerve  stimulation  are  at  the  most  weakened  or 
annulled  but  never  reversed.  The  explanation  was 
obtained  when  it  was  observed  that  the  action  of 
norepinephrine  on  the  arterial  pressure  is  not  re- 
versed but  only  diminished  by  doses  which  reverse 
the  action  of  epinephrine.  This  difference  has  been 
utilized  for  the  classification  of  the  adrenergic  neuro- 
transmittor  both  in  vitro  (124,  129)  and  in  vivo  (20,  48). 

The  identification  has  subsequently  been  con- 
firmed by  other  methods,  notably  by  paper  and 
column  chromatography,  allowing  separation  from 
other  catechol  amines  and  characterization  by 
specific  color  or  fluorescence  reactions.  Extracts  of 
heart  yield  fractions  on  column  chromatography 
which  show  the  same  R-value  as  pure  norepinephrine 
and  the  same  biological  actions  (55)-  A  particularly 
good  source  of  the  adrenergic  transmitter  is  the 
splenic  nerves,  from  which  norepinephrine  can  be 
separated  by  column  chromatography  and  identified 
by  location  and  by  analysis  of  the  active  fractions 
(fig.  6).  Venous  blood  from  the  spleen  collected  dur- 


ing stimulation  of  the  adrenergic  nerves  contains 
practically  pure  norepinephrine  (98,  108). 

The  effects  of  reflex  activation  of  sympathetic 
nerves  as  well  as  the  effects  of  direct  nerve  stimulation 
show  all  the  characteristics  of  norepinephrine  actions 
(9.  48,  52,  58)- 

The  release  of  an  active  substance  on  stimulation 
of  the  nerves  to  an  organ  does  not  necessarily  mean 
that  this  substance  is  the  corresponding  chemotrans- 
mitter.  In  the  experiments  of  Loewi  in  1921  it  is  likely 
that  the  effects  observed  were  due  to  released  epineph- 
rine, for  which  good  evidence  was  obtained  later  (84, 
124).  There  is  no  evidence,  however,  that  epineph- 
rine serves  as  adrenergic  nerve  transmitter  in  any 
animal.  In  the  frog  the  spleen  contains  chiefly  nor- 
epinephrine (105),  and  it  can  not  be  excluded  that 
the  epinephrine  released  on  sympathetic  nerve  stimu- 
lation originates  from  chromaffin  cells  and  not  from 
adrenergic  nerve  endings.  In  such  a  case  the  substance 
released  from  the  heart  (which  lacks  coronary  vessels 
in  the  frog)  is  not  a  neurotransmitter  proper  and  the 
mechanism  involved  would  be  analogous  to  the  re- 
lease of  epinephrine  from  the  suprarenals  on  stimula- 
tion of  its  preganglionic  nerves. 

Although  the  theory  of  Cannon  and  Rosenblueth 
concerning  the  two  sympathins  is  chiefly  of  historical 
interest  is  may  be  briefly  outlined  here.  [For  a  de- 
tailed discussion  see  Cannon  &  Rosenblueth  (20), 
and  Rosenblueth  (113).]  According  to  this  theory 
epinephrine  is  the  adrenergic  nerve  transmitter,  which 
on    reaching    the    target    cells   combines   with   some 


20  25  30 

NUMBER  OF  TUBE 

FIG.  6.  Column  chromatogram  of  extract  of  beef  splenic 
nerves  after  adsorption  on  aluminium  oxide  and  elution,  show- 
ing a  maximum  for  norepinephrine.  [From  von  Euler  & 
Lishajko  (132).] 


220 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


TABLE  I .  Norepinephrine  Content  of  Beef  Nervous  Tissue 

in  tig  per  gm  (129) 


Splenic  nerve 

8.5-,8.5 

Splanchnic  ner\e 

4 

Sympathetic  chain,  thoracic 

■!  •5-4-9 

Sympathetic  chain,  cervical 

0.6 

Mesenteric  nerve 

1-5-3 

Superior  cervical  ganglion 

I 

Saphenous  nerve 

0.2-1 

Phrenic  nerve 

0.15-0.25 

Vagus  nerve 

0. 1 

Spinal  cord 

0.  T 

Brain 

0 . 04-0 . 20 

cell  constituent  to  form  what  was  termed  inhibitory 
(I)  or  excitatory  (E)  sympathin  or  both.  These  find- 
ings are  readily  explained  on  the  assumption  that 
the  actions  observed  were  either  due  to  the  true 
adrenergic  neurotransmitter,  norepinephrine,  or  to 
epinephrine  released  from  other  sources,  presumably 
chromaffin  cells,  or  a  mixture  of  both,  as  suggested 
i)y  Bacq  in  1934  and  subsequently  proved  by  the 
demonstration  of  both  amines  in  autonomically  in- 
nervated organs  (126).  The  term  sympathin  should 
preferably  be  abandoned  in  the  physiological  litera- 
ture since  it  does  not  discriminate  between  the  neuro- 
transmitter and  the  hormones  released  as  a  result  of 
preganglionic  stimulation  of  chromaffin  cells. 


Occurrence,   Biosynthesis  and  Storage  oj   Adrenergic 
Nerve  Transmitter 

Unless  it  is  assumed  that  the  chemical  transmitters 
are  being  formed  and  released  at  the  moment  of 
nervous  excitation  it  must  be  concluded  that  they 
are  present  in  the  axon  and  relea.sed  from  .some  kind 
of  store.  Systematic  studies  of  the  content  of  trans- 
mitter substances  in  postganglionic  nerves  have  been 
made  both  for  the  cholinergic  and  for  the  adrenergic 
system.  Such  experiments  have  shown  that  the  content 
of  norepinephrine  in  a  nerve  correlates  well  with  the 
number  of  unmyelinated  fibers  of  autonomic  origin 
(i  1 1).  As  seen  in  table  i  the  amount  of  norepineph- 
rine varies  greatly  and  is  highest  in  the  splenic  nerves 
which  are  known  to  contain  practically  only  post- 
ganglionic sympathetic  fibers.  In  other  nerves,  such 
as  the  vagus,  the  amount  is  quite  small  and  this  is 
true  also  for  most  motor  nerves  and  the  majority  of 
sensory  nerves.  For  technical  reasons  it  is  impossible 
to  prepare  nerves  in  their  most  peripheral  parts, 
hence  it  has  not  been  possible  to  study  directly  the 
content  of  the  transmitters  in  the  immediate  vicinitv 


of  the  target  cell,  which  for  many  reasons  would 
have  been  desirable.  On  the  other  hand  it  has  been 
possible  partly  to  overcome  this  difficulty  by  making 
extracts  of  whole  organs  and  estimating  their  trans- 
mitter content,  thus  measuring  the  total  amount 
present  in  the  tissue  including  the  finest  nerve  rami- 
fications (iio,  129).  Proof  that  the  transmitter  sub- 
stances so  found  are  actually  due  to  the  presence  of 
postganglionic  nerve  fibers  has  been  obtained  by 
studying  the  effect  of  denervation.  If  the  postgangli- 
onic nerves  are  severed  and  allowed  to  degenerate, 
the  amount  of  norepinephrine  in  the  peripheral  ti.ssue 
falls  to  very  low  figures  or  disappears  completely. 
This  indicates  that  the  tissue  is  not  able  to  store  the 
transmitter  by  itself  but  does  so  by  means  of  its 
autonomic  nerve  fibers.  Further  support  for  this 
opinion  is  provided  by  experiments  showing  that 
some  4  to  8  weeks  after  degeneration  of  the  cardiac 
nerves  the  content  of  adrenergic  transmitter  in  the 
sheep  heart  increases  again  and  after  the  lapse  of  a 
few  months  reaches  the  original  value  (fig.  7)  (55). 
.Similar  results  have  been  oljtained  for  other  organs 


2P 

1,8 

D  left  cerv.  symp   removed 

^1.5 

ao 

ffl  right   "        " 

5l> 

e  lei!   stellate   removed 

21,2 

9  right     " 

■ 

%\o 

%Ofi 

fB(nofmal)               m 

iO!& 

■ 

|0,4 

■ 

0.2 
0 

•% 

■ 
■ 

1                2 

3               4                5               6 

lime-weeks  after   last  operation 

FIG.   7.  Norepinephrine  content  of  sheep  hearts  before  and 
various   times   after   svmpathetic   denervation.    [From   Goodall 

(55)-] 


T.\BLE  2.  Norepinephrine  Content  in  Beef  Organs 

in  fig  per  gm  (129) 


Spleen 

Lymph  glands 

Heart 

Ciliary  body  and  iris 

Liver 

Arteries  and  veins 

Lung 

Intestine 

Uterus 

Testicle 

Skeletal  muscle 

Bone  marrow 


1-5-3-5 

0.5-0.8 

o . 3-0 . 6 

0.4 

o .  25 

o.  I-I 

0.15 

0.15 
0.15 

0.04 
0.04 
0.0 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


221 


such  as  the  spleen  and  the  kidney  of  the  sheep  (129). 
The  stores  of  the  transmitter  substance  can  thus  be 
estimated  by  extracting  the  tissue  and  subjecting  it  to 
chemical  or  biological  analysis.  The  content  of 
adrenergic  transmitter  in  an  organ  (table  2)  provides 
a  measure  of  the  relative  supply  of  adrenergic  nerves. 
Norepinephrine  was  first  suggested  as  a  link  in  the 
biosynthesis  chain  leading  to  epinephrine  by  Blaschko 
(11).  The  basis  for  this  was  given  by  Holtz,  Heise  & 
Liidtke  (68)  who  discovered  an  enzyme  capable  of 
decarboxylating  levo-dihydroxyphenylalanine  (dopa) 
to  its  corresponding  amine,  hydroxytyramine  (dopa- 
mine). This  enzyme  was  present  in  liver  and  kidney 
and  has  also  been  demonstrated  in  the  adrenals  and 
in  adrenergic  nerves  (69).  While  it  has  been  shown 
experimentally  that  homogenates  of  the  adrenal  gland 
synthesize  norepinephrine  from  tyrosine  (74),  via 
dopa  (33)  and  dopamine  (59),  this  sequence  has  not 
been  shown  for  adrenergic  nerves  although  there 
can  be  little  doubt  that  this  is  the  case.  At  any  rate 
it  has  been  found  that  extracts  of  the  spleen  or 
splenic  nerves  contain  relatively  large  amounts  of 
dopamine  (117,  132).  The  biosynthesis  may  there- 
fore be  depicted  by  the  following  .scheme: 


OH 


OH 


iOH 


CH2CHCOOH 


CH.,CHCOOH 


NH., 

NH., 

Tyrosine 

Dopa 

0 

0 

H 
OH 

0 

A 

H 
OH 

CH.,CH2- 

NH2 

CHrCHOHNH 

D( 

jpamine 

No 

repinephrine 

It  appears  likely  that  the  biosynthesis  is  located  in 
the  place  of  storage  (see  below).  Analysis  of  extracts 
of  autonomic  nerves  have  shown  that  the  norepineph- 
rine content  is  a  function  of  the  proportion  of  adrener- 
gic fibers.  These  contain  the  transmitter  along  their 
whole  length  and  also  in  the  jxU  soma.  A  very 
marked  accumulation  in  the  terminal  parts  must  be 
assumed  for  the  following  reasons.  Splenic  nerves  of 
the  beef  contain  about  15  ng  norepinephrine  per  gm 
fresh  tissue  after  removal  of  the  sheath,  while  the 
content  of  the  whole  organ  is  about  3  //g  per  gm.  Since 


all  of  the  splenic  norepinephrine  disappears  on  section 
and  degeneration  of  the  adrenergic  nerves  to  the 
organ  it  is  assumed  that  the  norepinephrine  found  in 
the  organ  is  bound  to  its  nerves.  On  the  other  hand 
it  is  inconceivable  that  20  per  cent  of  the  splenic 
tissue  consists  of  nerves,  and  it  follows  from  this  that 
some  parts  of  the  nerves,  presumably  the  endings, 
contain  much  more  of  the  transmitter  than  the  main 
nerve  trunks. 

Even  after  rernoval  from  the  body,  organs  retaiin 
their  adrenergic  transmitter  substance  for  a  con- 
siderable time.  A  beef  spleen  may  thus  be  stored  at 
room  temperature  for  24  hours  without  any  detectable 
loss  of  norepinephrine.  This  indicates  that  it  is  not 
present  in  a  freely  diffusible  form  and  strongly  sug- 
gests that  it  is  bound  in  such  a  way  as  to  prevent  con- 
tact with  inactivating  enzymes. 

Evidence  has  been  obtained  for  the  storage  of  the 
hormones  of  chromaffin  cells  in  specific  granules  (12, 
63).  By  increasing  the  acidity  of  the  surrounding  solu- 
tion to  pH5  or  lower,  the  chromaffin  cell  hormones 
are  released  from  the  granules  (63).  When  the  same 
principle  was  applied  to  the  isolated  spleen  by  per- 
fusi^ng  it  with  a  .solution  containing  acids  such  as  as- 
corbic, citric  or  lactic  acid,  the  transmitter  substance 
was  released  and  could  be  demonstrated  in  the  per- 
fusion fluid  (40).  Also  other  substances  which  have 
been  found  effective  in  releasing  the  hormones  from 
isolated  granules  had  a  similar  action  on  the  perfused 
spleen,  such  as  detergents,  digitonin  and  lecithinase 
from  snake  venom. 

These  e.xperiments  add  support  to  the  hypothesis 
(127)  that  the  neurotransmitter  is  stored,  and  proba- 
bly manufactured,  in  specific  structures  in  the 
adrenergic  axon.  Experiments  by  Euler  &  Hillarp 
(131)  have  demonstrated  that  a  microgranular  frac- 
tion rich  in  norepinephrine  can  be  separated  by  high 
speed  centrifugation  from  a  homogenate  of  beef 
splenic  nerves.  The  chemotransmitter  is  apparently 
stored  in  elements  surrounded  by  a  membrane  since 
a  suspension  of  the  sediment  in  Ringer's  solution 
does  not  give  off  norepinephrine  to  the  surrounding 
fluid.  If  acid  is  added  to  PH4  in  the  suspension,  the 
norepinephrine  is  instantaneously  released,  however, 
and  can  be  demonstrated  by  ijiological  and  chemical 
methods  in  the  suspension  fluid.  The  micrograniilar 
stores  are  apparently  specific  for  the  chemotrans- 
mitter since  the  histamine  which  is  abundant  in  the 
beef  splenic  nerves  (about  100  ixg  per  gm  nerve)  is  not 
present  in  the  same  structural  elements.  Certain 
cellular  fractions  have  been  found  to  contain  more 
than   1.5  fig  norepinephrine  per  mg  dry  weight  or 


222 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


around  20  times  the  amount  per  mg  dry  weight  found 
in  the  whole  nerve  before  homogenization. 

The  theory  may  then  be  advanced  that  the  adrener- 
gic nerve  transmitter  is  bound  to  elements  which  in 
principle  are  of  a  kind  similar  to  those  found  in  the 
chromaffin  cells.  Since  these  can  be  regarded  as 
homologues  of  the  postganglionic  neurons  it  might 
be  expected  that  their  constituents  with  specific  activ- 
ity are  stored  in  a  similar  way.  The  structural  ele- 
ments serving  as  stores  may  also  well  be  the  units  for 
bio.synthesis.  Apparently  this  takes  place  very  rapidly 
so  as  to  maintain  a  practically  constant  store.  Con- 
tinuous and  prolonged  stimulation  of  nerves  in  vitro 
(88)  or  in  vivo  (129)  does  not  deplete  the  stores.  There 
is  no  evidence  that  the  granules  of  the  chromaffin 
cells  leave  the  cell  body  in  connection  with  the  re- 
lease of  the  hormones;  this  may  be  assumed  also  for 
the  storing  elements  of  the  postganglionic  adrenergic 
neurons.  It  may  be  postulated  that  the  microstruc- 
tures  elaborating  and  containing  the  neurotrans- 
mitter are  formed  in  the  cell  soma  and  transported 
along  the  axon  towards  the  periphery  by  the  axo- 
plasm  flow  (135).  These  assumptions  would  provide 
a  satisfactory  explanation  for  the  findings  that  a)  the 
chemotransmitter  is  accumulated  in  the  terminal  parts 
of  the  neuron,  and  that  h~)  continuous  stimulation  does 
not  deplete  the  nerves  of  its  chemotransmitter.  The 
theory  involving  the  assumption  of  intra-axonal 
microstructural  elements  thus  seems  to  explain 
several  phenomena  encountered  in  the  field  of  neuro- 
transmission. 

Release 

Stimulation  of  the  adrenergic  nerves,  either  directly 
or  reflexly,  immediately  releases  norepinephrine 
which  is  then  allowed  to  diffuse  to  the  adjacent  tar- 
get cells.  From  the  above  section  it  may  be  inferred 
that  the  transmitter  is  released  from  microstructures 
and  accumulated  at  the  terminal  parts  of  the  nerves, 
presumably  in  a  way  similar  to  that  operating  in  the 
chromaffin  cells.  The  large  number  of  discrete 
terminal  ramifications  ofl"er  only  short  diffusion  dis- 
tances, enabling  each  cell  to  be  reached  by  the  chemi- 
cal transmitter  in  a  very  short  time.  While  under 
normal  conditions  the  adrenergic  chemotransmitter 
is  released  chiefly,  if  not  entirely,  as  a  result  of  reflex 
stimulation,  various  experimental  approaches  have 
been  made  in  order  to  study  the  release  in  more 
detail,  such  as  a)  observation  of  the  effects  of  direct 
nerve  stimulation  on  the  innervated  organ.  A)  record- 
ing of  the  effects  of  stimulation  of  adrenergic  nerves 


on  remote  organs,  c)  quantitatixe  estimation  of  the 
content  of  the  neurotransmitter  in  the  venous  effluent 
from  the  stimulated  organ,  and  (T)  measuring  the 
release  of  transmitter  from  isolated  nerves  stimulated 
in  vitro,  or  from  organs  perfused  in  vitro. 

INFLUENCE  OF  STIMUL.XTION  FREQUENCY.    The   effect   of 

Stimulation  of  the  adrenergic  nerves — or  usually 
mixed  nerves  containing  adrenergic  fibers — provides 
the  basis  for  most  of  our  knowledge  of  the  action  of 
the  adrenergic  system  on  various  target  organs.  A 
study  of  these  effects  not  only  permits  qualitative  in- 
formation on  the  type  of  effect  on  the  organ  but  also 
offers  opportunities  for  gaining  quantitative  infor- 
mation, for  instance  about  the  influence  of  stimulus 
strength  and  frequency  on  the  effect.  In  this  way  the 
relea.se  mechanism  can  be  studied  at  least  on  a  semi- 
quantitative basis  which  can  hardly  be  accom- 
plished  by  reflex  stimulation. 

While  the  technique  of  studying  the  response  of  an 
organ  to  variation  in  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus 
gives  an  idea  of  the  excitability  of  the  nerve  fibers, 
information  about  the  release  mechanism  is  better 
obtained  by  varying  the  stimulus  frequency.  Such 
experiments  are  preferably  performed  u.sing  stimu- 
lation intensities  which  will  allow  participation  of 
all  fibers.  As  shown  in  figure  8,  the  curves  obtained 
by  Rosenblueth  (112)  showing  the  relationship  of 
stimulus  frequency  and  effect  on  various  autonomic 
effectors  have  the  general  shape  of  rectangular 
hyperbolas.  The  results  show  the  noteworthy  feature 
that  considerable  effects  are  achieved  even  at  very 
low  frequencies.  As  can  be  seen  from  figure  8,  e\en 
frequencies  of  less  than  i  per  sec.  are  capable  of 
causing  marked  effects.  Nearly  maximal  actions  have 
been  recorded  with  frequencies  of  the  order  of  5  per 
sec,  for  instance  on  the  piloerectors  and  the  nictitat- 
ing membrane.  The  results  imply  that  even  very  low 
frequencies  are  sufficient  to  release  considerable 
amounts  of  the  transmitter.  In  table  3  the  optimum 
frequencies  for  a  numjjer  of  effectors  are  given.  Maxi- 
mal effects  are  obtained  with  frequencies  varying 
from  20  to  30  per  sec.  in  most  effector  systems.  Even 
a  frequency  of  10  per  sec.  generally  elicits  more  than 
80  per  cent  of  the  maximal  response. 

.\n  interesting  difference  is  noted  between  the 
ratio  of  the  effects  of  single  stimuli  and  those  of 
maximal  tetanic  stimuli  on  smooth  and  skeletal  mus- 
cles, no  doubt  depending  on  the  trigger  mechanism  in 
the  latter.  Thus  the  ratio  between  the  effects  is  much 
higher  for  the  smooth  muscle  than  for  the  skeletal 
muscle. 


AUTONOMIC   NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


223 


FIG.  8.  Frequency-response  curves  of  sympathetic  effectors.  .-1:  abscissae,  frequencies  of  stimulation 
of  tlie  lumbar  sympathetics;  ordinates,  angles  of  erection  of  a  hair  in  the  tail  of  a  cat.  B:  abscissae, 
frequencies  of  stimulation  of  the  cervical  sympathetic;  ordinates,  heights  of  the  records  of  isotonic 
contractions  of  the  nictitating  membrane  15  sec.  after  the  beginning  of  stimulation.  C:  as  in  B,  but 
isometric  contractions  of  the  nictitating  membrane.  D :  abscissae,  frequencies  of  stimulation  of  the 
right  cardioaccelerator  nerves;  ordinates,  maximal  increases  of  heart  rate  per  15  sec.  [From  Rosen- 
blueth  (112).] 


Even  after  cutting  a  considerable  portion  of  the 
nerve  the  maximal  effect  may  be  approached,  pro- 
vided the  frequency  of  stimulation  is  increased  suffi- 
ciently. The  effect  of  low  frequencies  on  a  partially 
severed  nerve  is  smaller  than  in  the  intact  nerve, 
however,  which  might  be  expected. 

The  conclusion  drawn  from  these  experiments  is 
that  the  neurotransmitter  diffuses  to  the  neighboring 
cells  as  its  concentration  is  raised  by  increasing  the 
stimulation  frequency.  The  principle  of  activation  of 
smooth  muscle  cells  may  therefore  be  a  general  re- 
lease of  transmitter  within  the  mass  of  these  cells, 
rnaking  individual  innervation  as  for  the  skeletal 
muscle  fibers  unnecessary. 


T.'VBLE  3.  Frequencies  of  Preganglionic  Stimulation, 
Giving  Maximal  Response  of  Effectors  (20) 


Effectors 
Sympathetic 

Pilomotors 

Nictitating  membrane 

Pregnant  uterus 

Intestine 

Adrenal  medulla 

Heart  (postgangl.) 
Parasympathetic 

Heart 

Submaxillary  gland 

Stomach 


Frequency 
Stim.  per  sec. 


■5 
20 
20 
20 
25 
25 


30 
35 
25 


224 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


MAX  CONSTRICTION 
100 


BASAL 
BLOOO   FLOW 

PHYSIOL  DISCHARGE  RANGE 


JSTIM 
30    FREQ 


FIG.  9-  Vasoconstrictor  effect  of  electric  stimulation  of  lumbar  sympathetics  at  \arying  frequencies 
in  the  cat.  Striped  area  indicates  the  variations  obser\'ed  in  40  experiments.  A  represents  a\'erage  of 
10  experiments  with  the  biggest  response;  B,  average  response  after  vasodilator  drugs.  [From  Folkow 
(46).] 


The  effect  of  stimulation  at  various  frequencies  of 
sympathetic  nerves  to  the  muscular  Ijlood  vessels  in 
the  lower  part  of  the  hind  limlj  of  the  cat  has  been 
measured  by  recording  the  outflow  (46). 

Figure  9  shows  the  correlation  between  stimula- 
tion rate  and  the  constrictor  response.  It  is  clearly 
seen  that  low  stimulation  frequencies  are  very  effec- 
tive. This  applies  also  to  cutaneous  blood  vessels  (22). 

A  detailed  analysis  of  the  mechanism  of  the  release 
has  been  made  by  Brown  &  Gillespie  (14)  using  the 
cat's  spleen.  Samples  of  venous  blood  were  collected 
and  the  norepinephrine  content  assayed  on  the  ar- 
terial pressure  of  the  pithed  rat.  Supramaximal 
stimuli  were  applied  to  the  splenic  nerve,  the  total 
number  of  stimuli  being  200,  irrespective  of  the  fre- 
quencv.  Both  adrenal  glands  were  removed  and  the 
splanchnic  nerves  cut.  The  output  of  norepinephrine 
was  expressed  as  amount  released  per  stimulus. 

As  illustrated  in  figure  io.4  the  norepinephrine  out- 
put per  stimulus  was  low  at  low  frequencies,  but  as 
the  frequency  increased  the  amount  found  in  venous 
blood  rose  sharply  to  a  maximum  at  about  30  stimu- 
lations per  sec.  Since  the  output  per  stimulus  was 
the  same  before  and  after  addition  of  isopropyl 
isonicotinyl  hydrazine  (Marsilid),  an  effect  of  amine 
oxidase  on  the  transmitter  liberated  at  lower  frequen- 
cies could  be  excluded.  The  possibility  was  also  dis- 
cussed that,  although  the  amount  of  transmitter  re- 
leased by  each  nerve  volley  might  be  constant,  more 
was  'utilized'  by  tissue  receptors  at  a  low  rate  of  stim- 


ulation. After  blocking  tissue  receptors  with  N-N- 
dibenzyl-/3-chloroethylamine  (dibenamine),  it  was 
found  that  the  output  per  impulse  reaching  the  blood 
was  greatly  increased  at  the  lower  frequencies  and 
maintained  a  constant  value  at  different  frequencies. 
From  these  observations  it  was  concluded  that  the 
norepinephrine  release  per  nerve  volley  is  constant 
and  that  the  fraction  removed  by  the  tissues  is 
greater  at  the  lower  frequencies  of  stimulation  (cf. 
section  on  removal  of  transmitter,  p.  227). 

The  experiments  quoted  above  may  have  an  in- 
teresting implication  in  that  the  small  or  absent  over- 
flow at  low  stimulation  frequency  (or  adrenergic 
nerve  activity)  and  the  larger  overflow  at  higher  ac- 
tivity may  cause  an  excretion  pattern  in  the  urine 
which  'amplifies'  the  actual  release  and  makes  differ- 
ences more  pronounced  than  would  be  expected  from 
the  activity  of  the  effector. 

EFFECTS  ON  REMOTE  ORGANS.  This  method  of  study- 
ing the  release  of  the  adrenergic  transmitter  is  the 
one  which  led  to  the  discovery  and  demonstration  of 
such  a  mechanism.  The  first  experiments  of  this  kind 
were  made  by  Cannon  &  Uridil  in  1921  (21)  who  ob- 
served the  effect  of  stimulating  the  li\cr  nerves  on  the 
heart  and  iris  .sensitized  by  denervation.  They  as- 
cribed the  effect  to  a  "special  and  unknown  sub- 
stance" apparently  being  set  free  by  the  stimulation. 
This  kind  of  experiment  was  developed  further  by 
Cannon  and  Rosenblueth  and  their  co-workers  in  the 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


12 


0-8 


0  4 


Frequency  (stimuli/eec.) 


J I I L 


I        I        I        I I 


0 
12   - 


0-8 


20 


40 


60 


80  100 


200        800 


0  4 


Frequency  (per  sec.) 


10 


2U 


30 


40 


50 


FIG.  lo.  --1;  Mean  output  per  stimulus  of  'sympathin'  plotted 
against  the  frequency  of  stimulation.  At  all  frequencies  of 
stimulation  the  total  number  of  pulses  was  2oq.  The  vertical 
lines  represent  the  standard  errors  of  the  means.  Figures  for 
loo,  200  and  300  pulses  per  sec.  are  single  observations.  B: 
First  part  of  the  graph  in  A  with  an  extended  scale  for  frequency. 
The  individual  results  from  three  animals  previously  given 
dibenamine  are  shown.  The  output  per  stimulus  at  10  pulses 
per  sec.  has  increased  and  equals  the  maximum  in  the  un- 
treated animal.  There  is  no  obvious  variation  with  frequency. 
[From  Brown  &  Gillespie  (14).] 


work  on  'sympathin'.  While  the  study  of  the  trans- 
mitter release  in  this  manner,  by  recording  the  effect 
on  sensitized  remote  target  organs,  was  valuable  in 
the  elucidation  of  the  transmission  mechanism  as 
such,  its  physiological  significance  is  doubtful.  Even 
though  Cannon  and  Rosenblueth  and  their  co- 
workers obtained  increases  in  heart  rate,  dilatation  of 
the  pupil  and  contraction  of  the  nictitating  membrane 
in  denervated  organs  after  stimulation  of  sympathetic 
nerves  in  other  parts  of  the  body,  the  appearance  of 
remote  effects  caused  by  transportation  of  the  re- 
leased transmitter  by  the  blood  is  by  no  means  a 
constant  phenomenon. 


The  failure  of  some  authors  (22)  to  observe  remote 
effects  even  on  the  highly  sensitized  denervated 
nictitating  membrane  in  spite  of  intense  stimulation 
of  sympathetic  nerves  has  been  taken  to  indicate  the 
presence  of  peripheral  inactivation  mechanisms  which 
largely  eliminate  an  overflow  of  transmitter.  How- 
ever, a  physiologically  occurring  overflow  in  the 
meaning  of  Cannon  and  Rosenblueth  cannot  be 
denied  for  the  following  reason.  If  the  catechol  amines 
are  estimated  in  urine  from  adrenalectomized  pa- 
tients the  amounts  of  epinephrine  are  very  low  while 
the  norepinephrine  content  tends  to  be  even  higher 
than  in  normal  subjects  (129).  The  only  possibility 
for  norepinephrine  to  occur  in  the  urine  then  is  a 
release  from  some  source  in  the  body  other  than  the 
adrenals.  Since  the  adrenergic  nerves  are  known  to 
contain  large  amounts  of  this  transmitter,  it  appears 
legitimate  to  assume  that  during  the  incessant  ac- 
tivity of  the  adrenergic  system  a  certain  overflow  of 
transmitter  takes  place  continuously. 

As  to  the  value  of  the  remote  effects  studied  by 
Cannon  and  Rosenblueth  as  a  proof  of  chemotrans- 
mission  from  nerves,  it  should  be  borne  in  mind  that 
nervous  stimulation  might  also  cause  a  release  from 
chrornaffin  cells  present  in  the  tissues.  This  criticism 
does  not  invalidate  their  conclusions  in  principle 
since  there  is  good  evidence  in  some  of  Cannon  and 
Rosenblueth's  experiments  that  at  least  some  of  the 
effects  are  due  to  the  release  of  norepinephrine. 

It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the  so-called  inhibitory 
sympathin  is  obtained  when  the  splanchnics  are 
stimulated  but  not  when  the  hepatic  nerves  are  stimu- 
lated (fig.  1 1).  It  is  known  that  the  s£lanchnic_nerves 
may  innervate  groups  of  chromaitiin-cells^at  various 
sites.  Their  secretory  products  may  then  be  carried 
by  the  blood  stream  to  excite  the  denervated  organ. 
In  case  of  the  hepatic  nerves  there  was  only  a  stim- 
ulating effect  but  no  inhibitory  effect  on  the  dener- 
vated uterus  of  the  cat,  indicating  that  practically  only 
nore£ine£hrine  was  released  in  this  case.  As  far  as 
can  be  ascertained  at  the  present  time  this  norepi- 
nephrine is  released  from  adrenergic  nerve  endings. 

The  question  whether  reflex  liberation  of  the 
adrenergic  transmitter  could  be  large  enough  to 
cause  actions  on  remote  organs  has  al.so  been  studied 
(80).  As  a  result  of  afferent  sciatic  or  brachial  nerve 
stimulation  it  was  possible  to  demonstrate  a  contrac- 
tion of  the  denervated  nictitating  membrane  in  the 
adrenalectomized  cat.  It  has  also  been  possible  to 
show  a  reflex  liberation  of  the  adrenergic  transmitter 
by  action  on  remote  organs,  for  instance  after  excite- 
ment and  struggle  (103).  The  slower  development  of 


■226 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  1 1 .  Upper  curves,  decentralized  nictitating  membrane; 
■lower  curves,  denervated  nonpregnant  uterus  of  the  cat.  Con- 
traction upwards.  Time,  30  sec.  A:  hepatic  nerves  stimulated. 
B:  right  splanchnic  nerves  stimulated  after  exclusion  of  ad- 
renals. C:  same  as  in  B  and  injection  of  1.5  Mg  epinephrine. 
D:  duodenohepatic  nerves  stimulated.  E:  same  as  in  D  after 
severance  of  duodenal  nerves.  [From  Cannon  &  Rosenblueth 
(19)-] 


the  blood-borne  action  on  the  denervated  heart  after 
reflex  excitation,  resuhing  from  struggle,  as  compared 
with  the  rapid  and  large  effect  in  cases  where  the 
adrenals  were  active  may  be  explained  by  the  gradual 
and  prolonged  release  of  moderate  amounts  of  trans- 
mitter in  the  former  case.  The  activation  of  the 
adrenals  has  a  tendency  to  cause  an  'explosive'  re- 
lease. A  continuous  liberation  into  the  blood  stream 
during  'sham  rage'  has  also  been  noted  (136)  in 
decorticate  cats  showing  a  quasiemotional  state  as 
exidenced  by  the  reduction  in  the  rate  of  the  dener- 
vated heart  after  section  of  the  hepatic  nerves.  Even 
as  a  consequence  of  normal  emotions  a  release  of 
transmitter  into  the  blood  has  been  observed. 

On  exposure  to  cold  no  sign  of  continuous  effect  on 
the  denervated  nictitating  membrane  of  the  cat  was 
found,  however  (107).  The  persistent  erection  of  hairs 
when  the  animal  is  in  cold  surroundings  is  apparently 
not  accompanied  by  a  liberation  of  enough  trans- 
mitter to  affect  remote  organs  even  if  these  are  sensi- 
tized. 

During^ypoglycemia  there  is  no  evidence  for  ac- 
tivation of  the  sympathetic  system  as  a  whole.  Only 
a  selective  release  of  epinephrine  from  the  suprarenal 
has  been  demonstrated  by  direct  analysis  of  the 
venous  blood  (38).  The  contention  expressed  by  Can- 
non and  Rosenblueth  that  "it  is  characteristic  of  the 


sympathetic  system,  when  specially  excited,  to  act  as 
a  whole;  thus  adrenine  is  secreted  by  splanchnic  im- 
pulses at  the  same  time  that  sympathetic  impulses 
elsewhere  in  the  body  are  liberating  sympathin"  has 
not  been  corroborated  by  later  experiments  and 
experience.  It  is  now  reasonably  certain  that  the 
secretion  of  epinephrine  is  a  process  which  occurs  in- 
dependently and  often  during  quite  other  conditions 
than  the  activation  of  other  parts  of  the  sympathetic 
system.  It  would  also  appear  peculiar  if  the  action  of 
epinephrine  in  maintaining  blood  sugar  homeostasis 
should  be  obligatorily  linked  with,  for  instance,  a  rise 
of  arterial  pressure  as  a  result  of  generalized  adrener- 
gic activity.  The  statement  of  Cannon  and  Rosen- 
blueth that  "adrenine  and  sympathin  collaborate  in 
affecting  structures  innerv-ated  by  sympathetic 
nerves"  is  only  true  in  a  restricted  sense  and  its  biolog- 
ical significance  is  too  limited  to  be  set  forth  as  a  gen- 
eral rule.  The  statement  also  illustrates  the  hazards 
of  using  the  term  'sympathin'  since  this  may  repre- 
sent either  epinephrine  or  norepinephrine.  It  may  be 
recalled  that  the  two  amines  have  opposite  effects  for 
instance  on  the  vessels  of  the  skeletal  muscles  (i ,  8,  37). 
Even  if  the  leakage  of  transmitter  into  the  blood  stream 
is  negligible  from  the  point  of  view  of  physiological 
action,  this  phenomenon  has  been  of  great  heuristic 
value  as  in  Cannon  and  Rosenblueth's  work  and  also 
in  the  extensive  work  dealing  with  the  excretion  of 
the  neurotransmitter  in  urine  (67,  129). 

Information  about  the  nerv-e  transmitter  may  also 
be  gained  by  collecting  blood  or  perfusing  fluid  from 
an  organ  during  stimulation  of  the  sympathetic 
nerves,  and  by  recording  the  effects  of  this  fluid  on 
suitable  test  organs.  Studies  of  this  kind  are  in  prin- 
ciple similar  to  the  pioneer  experiments  by  Loewi. 
Active  substances  in  the  effluent  have  been  demon- 
strated in  many  instances,  such  as  from  the  frog's 
stomach  (13),  the  aqueous  humour  (3),  the  rabbit's 
intestine  (45)  and  the  dog's  tongue  (6). 

By  the  use  of  an  appropriate  testing  technique  it 
could  be  shown  later  that  the  active  substance  re- 
leased by  adrenergic  nerve  stimulation  conformed 
in  its  properties  with  norepinephrine  (14,  93,  98,  106, 
108).  In  these  studies  the  venous  plasma  of  the  stimu- 
lated organ  was  tested.  Most  investigators  have  also 
stated  that  smaller  amoujits  of  epinephrine  were 
sometimes  also  liberated.  The  significance  of  the 
simultaneous  appearance  of  small  amounts  of  epi- 
nephrine will  be  considered  below. 

The  release  of  epinephrine-like  materials  on  stimu- 
lation of  the  vagus  nerve  to  the  atropinized  heart  has 
also  been  reported  (65,  94).  The  former  authors  con- 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


227 


eluded  that  the  epinephrine-Hke  substance  was  re- 
leased from  intracardiac  adrenergic  neurons  con- 
trolled bv  preganglionic  fibers  in  the  vagus.  Whether 
the  substance  was  released  from  neurons  proper  or 
from  chromaffin  cells  is  not  clear,  however. 

STIMULATION  OF  ISOLATED  NERVES.  Attempts  havc  been 
made  to  study  the  release  of  the  adrenergic  trans- 
mitter by  stimulating  isolated  nerves,  thus  avoiding 
the  possibility  of  interaction  of  the  inner\'ated  tissues. 
In  unpublished  experiments  Gaddum  &  Khayyal 
(50)  stimulated  an  isolated  sympathetic  nerve  sus- 
pended in  salt  solution  and  found  that  a  sympathomi- 
metic substance  was  released  into  the  solution.  This 
efTect  was  later  attributed  to  damage  to  the  nerve  by 
the  stimulating  electrodes  (53).  However,  the  original 
finding  was  later  confirined  (79).  This  is  in  agreement 
with  the  fact  that  the  whole  nerve  trunk  contains 
norepinephrine. 

EXHAUSTIBILITY.  Studies  on  the  exhaustibility  of  the 
transmitter  sources  have  shown  that  even  prolonged 
stimulation,  reflex  or  direct,  does  not  seem  to  lessen 
the  release.  Orias  (104)  stimulated  the  preganglionic 
fibers  of  the  cervical  sympathetic  10  times  a  sec.  for 
I  hour  and  found  no  .signs  of  fatigue  in  the  responses 
of  the  nictitating  membrane.  These  experiments  were 
repeated  by  Dye  (39)  who  applied  not  less  than 
108,000  Stimuli  during  3  hours  to  the  preganglionic 
nerves  without  evidence  of  exhaustion.  Luco  &  Goni 
(88)  found  that  stimulation  of  sympathetic  nerves 
for  I  hour  did  not  diminish  the  content  of  transmitter 
in  the  nerve.  It  may  therefore  be  assumed  that  release 
of  the  transmitter  can  continue  for  an  unlimited  time. 
This  is  an  indication  in  the  first  place  that  the  trans- 
mitter is  readily  resynthesized  but  also  that  the 
release  mechanism  is  built  to  render  continuous 
service. 

Removal  of  Transmitter 

Although  it  is  apparent  from  the  observations  of 
remote  effects  of  adrenergic  nerve  stimulation  and 
from  the  excretion  of  norepinephrine  in  urine  that  a 
certain  proportion  of  the  released  neurotransmitter 
is  transferred  into  the  circulating  blood,  it  is  generally 
assumed  that  most  of  the  transmitter  is  being  inacti- 
vated at  or  near  the  site  of  release  (16,  47}. 

The  experiments  of  Brown  &  Gillespie  (14)  indicate 
that  the  removal  of  the  transmitter  is  more  efficient 
when  it  is  released  at  a  slow  rate.  As  to  the  mechanism 
of  removal,  their  experiments  suggest  that  the  trans- 


mitter is  being  attached  to  a  certain  extent  to  the 
effector  cells  and  presumably  inactivated  at  this  site. 

Our  knowledge  about  the  mechanism  of  inactiva- 
tion  is  still  very  incoinplete.  The  inability  of  iso- 
propyl  isonicotinyl  hydrazine  (Marsilid)  to  affect  to 
any  noticeable  extent  the  amount  of  transmitter  re- 
covered in  the  effluent  blood  after  stimulation  of  the 
splenic  nerves  does  not  support  the  common  opinion 
that  amine  oxiclase  plays  an  important  part  in  this 
respect. 

In  experiments  in  which  the  transmitter  was  re- 
leased from  a  perfused  spleen  by  various  chemical 
means,  the  amount  of  norepinephrine  found  in  the 
effluent  was  not  greatly  influenced  by  adding  amine 
oxidase  inhibitors  to  the  perfusion  fluid  (129).  More- 
over, administration  of  Marsilid  to  an  animal  does 
not  augment  the  degree  or  duration  of  adrenergic 
reflex  actions  in  the  cat,  such  as  the  pressor  eflfect  of 
carotid  occlusion,  indicating  that  amine  oxidase,  at 
any  rate,  does  not  attack  the  transmitter  between  the 
moment  of  release  and  the  action  on  the  efTector  cell. 

The  problem  of  the  removal  of  the  transmitter 
after  its  release  may  be  regarded  from  two  aspects. 
One  part  of  the  transmitter  apparently  is  d]rectly 
at_tached  to  the  effector  cells  [or 'utilized'  (14)]  while 
another  portion  is  leaking  into  the  blood  vessels,  or 
by-passing  the  target  cells  as  it  were.  It  is  conceivable 
that  after  saturation  of  the  target  cells  the  remainder 
of  the  released  transmitter  diffuses  through  the  capil- 
lary wall  and  enters  the  blood  stream.  The  situation 
might  be  regarded  as  analogous  to  that  prevailing 
during  reabsorption  of  a  threshold  substance  by  the 
renal  tubules  where  an  excess  causes  an  '  overflow' 
into  the  urine.  If  the  amount  of  the  transmitter  which 
is  caught  by  the  effector  cells  is  considered  first,  it 
appears  probable  that  it  is  being  inactivated  by  some 
process  so  far  unknown.  It  may  well  be  that  on  many 
occasions  this  part  represents  the  greatest  part  of  the 
released  transmitter.  The  second  part  which  is  not 
taken  up  by  the  cells  may  theoretically  be  attacked 
by  enzymes  on  its  diffusion  way  to  the  blood  or 
lymph  capillaries.  Apparently  this  is  not  the  case  since 
amine  oxidase  inhibitors  did  not  appreciably  alter 
the  yield  in  the  effluent  Ijlood  (14).  Not  even  after 
having  reached  the  blood  stream  is  the  inactivation 
complete  as  seen  by  the  excretion  in  urine  of  neuro- 
transmitter which  undoubtedly  originates  in  adre- 
nergic nerves,  as  indicated  by  the  excretion  in 
adrenalectomized  patients.  Knowing  the  proportion 
of  norepinephrine  e.xcreted  in  urine  after  intravenous 
infusion  at  a  constant  rate,  it  seems  po.ssible  to  obtain 
an  idea  of  the  '  overflow'   of  adrenergic    transmitter 


228 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


per  unit  of  time.  From  infusion  experiments  in  m^n 
it  has  been  found  that  the  proportion  of  norepi- 
nephrine excreted  in  urine  is  1.5  to  4  per  cent  of  that 
infused  durinc;  the  same  time  (129).  If  the  total  ex- 
cretion of  norepinephrine  (free  and  conjugated)  in 
man  during  24  hours,  when  the  subject  is  performing 
daily  routine  work  but  no  severe  muscular  work,  can 
be  estimated  at  60  fxg  (109,  129),  the  amount  of 
neurotransmitter  overflow  may  be  estimated  at  ap- 
proximately 2  to  3  mg  per  24  hours. 

The  careful  study  of  the  distribution  of  monoamine 
oxidase  (MAO)  in  various  nerve  cells  by  Koelle  & 
Valk  (76)  does  not  support  the  opinion  that  MAO  is 
specifically  occurring  in  adrenergic  nerves,  since  no 
significant  differences  were  found  in  the  MAO  ac- 
tivity in  nerve  cell  bodies  and  fibers  of  the  stellate, 
superior  cervical,  nodose,  dorsal  root  and  ciliary 
ganglia  of  the  cat.  The  enzyme  is  localized  in  smooth 
muscle  cells  of  blood  vessels.  It  is  absent  in  cardiac 
muscle,  but  high  activity  is  found  in  renal  tubule 
cells  and  hepatic  cells.  Since  the  removal  of  the  trans- 
mitter by  inactivating  enzymes  is  more  likely  to  occur 
in  the  target  cells  than  in  the  neurons  producing  the 
transmitter  this  result  is  not  unexpected. 

Small  amounts  of  the  transmitter  are  successfully 
removed  during  the  passage  of  blood  through  the 
tissue,  up  to  90  per  cent  during  a  single  passage 
through  muscle  and  skin.  This  is  in  harmony  with 
the  findings  that  after  infusion  of  norepinephrine  and 
epinephrine  in  man  only  a  small  percentage  appears 
in  the  urine,   the  rest  being  inactivated. 

Mechanisms  of  inactivation  other  than  by  MAO 
are  conceivable,  such  as  by  catechol  oxidases  and 
peroxidases  and  by  conjugation.  The  relative  unim- 
portance of  the  inactivation  of  circulating  catechol 
amines  by  MAO  is  further  borne  out  by  the  observa- 
tion that  cobefrine  (a-methyl-rfZ-norepinephrine)  is 
excreted  in  a  similar  small  proportion  as  epinephrine 
and  norepinephrine  after  injection  in  man  (129), 
although  it  is  not  attacked  by  this  enzyme.  It  must 
therefore  have  been  inactivated  (to  more  than  90  per 
cent)  by  some  other  mechanism  which  presumably 
would  have  been  similarly  active  on  the  catechol 
hormones. 


POSSIBLE   ADRENERGIC  NERVE   TRANSMITTERS 
OTHER    THAN    NOREPINEPHRINE 

It  may  well  be  asked  whether  there  is  any  way  of 
distinguishing  between  the  release  of  the  chemotrans- 


mitter  from  the  nerve  terminals  and  the  secretory 
products  from  chromaffin  cells  in  the  tissues.  Since 
very  little  is  known  about  the  mass  and  distribution 
of  such  scattered  chromaffin  cells  or  whether  they 
secrete  epinephrine  or  norepinephrine  or  both  (and 
in  the  latter  case  the  relative  proportions),  it  is  hard 
to  evaluate  the  amount  of  neurotransmitter  sensu 
strictwn  which  is  released  upon  stimulation  of  svinpa- 
thetic  nerves.  Assuming  that  chromaffin  cells  are 
present  in  an  organ,  they  would  be  made  to  release 
their  secretory  products  by  stimulation  of  the  pre- 
ganglionic fil:)ers  in  the  sympathetic  nerve. 

A  partial  answer  to  this  problem  has  been  afTorded 
by  studies  on  the  content  of  the  active  catechol 
amines  in  tissues  and  organs.  There  is  good  evidence 
that  the  catechol  amines  found  in  extracts  of  organs 
and  tissues  are  derived  from  their  adrenergic  nerves 
and  chromaffin  cells.  This  is  shown  by  a)  the  large 
reduction  or  disappearance  of  the  catechol  amines 
after  postganglionic  denervation  (18,  55,  129),  h)  the 
absence  of  these  suijstances  in  the  nerve-free  placenta 
(i  16,  124)  and  c)  the  reappearance  of  such  substances 
upon  regeneration  of  the  postganglionic  nerves 
(55,  129).  It  is  known  that  section  and  degeneration 
of  the  preganglionic  fibers  that  innervate  the  chro- 
maffin cells  do  not  cause  depletion  of  the  secretory 
products  of  these  cells,  while  section  of  the  postgangli- 
onic fibers  causes  disappearance  of  their  transmitter 
substance.  It  is  thus  possible  by  analysis  of  the  cate- 
chol amine  content  of  an  organ  after  preganglionic 
and  postganglionic  denervation  to  obtain  information 
on  the  occurrence  of  chromaffin  cells.  The  results  of 
such  experiments  liave  been  that  '  postganglionic' 
nerve  section  usually  leaves  a  small  remnant  of 
activity.  It  is  typical  of  this  that  the  proportion  of 
epinephrine  is  higher  than  it  is  in  the  organ  with  its 
nerves  intact  (55,  129).  Sometimes  the  epinephrine 
content  is  unchanged.  The  conclusion  has  been 
drawn  from  these  experiments  that_gractic_ally  all  of 
the  norepinephrine_is  present  in  the  postganglionic 
nerves  while  the  epinephrine  must  have  been  located 
outside  the  adrenergic  neurons,  in  all  likelihood  in 
chrouKiffin^cells.  Such  cells  have  been  described  in  the 
heart  b\  Trinci  (123). 

Further  evidence  along  the  same  line  has  been  ob- 
tained from  experiments  on  the  isolated  perfused 
rabbit  heart  either  beating  spontaneously  or  dri\'en 
electrically  at  a  faster  rate  (32).  By  recirculation  of 
the  perfusing  fluid  it  is  possible  to  concentrate  the 
active  substances  released  from  the  heart.  After  sepa- 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


229 


ration  by  chromatography  and  biological  estimation 
on  the  rat's  arterial  pressure  the  following  results 
(expressed  as  micrograms  per  heart  in  40  min.)  were 
obtained. 

Electrically  drisen 
Xorepinephrine  Epinephrine 

Mean:  0.01    ±  o.oi  0.08  ±  0.02 

Spontaneously  beating 
Mean:  0.02   ±  0.0 1  0.08  ±  0.02 

These  results  are  of  interest  since  they  clearly  show 
that  the  proportion  of  epinephrine  is  far  higher  than 
that  occurring  in  extracts  of  hearts  or  in  the  coronary 
blood  plasma  after  stimulation  of  cardiac  sympa- 
thetic nerves  (129).  The  reason  for  the  large  release 
of  epinephrine  in  the  spontaneously  beating  heart  is 
obscure,  howe\'er.  It  therefore  appears  justified  to 
conclude  that  the  epinephrine  released  probably 
originates  from  chromaffin  cells.  On  the  other  hand 
the  norepinephrine  left  in  an  organ  after  postgangli- 
onic denervation  constitutes  such  a  small  part  of  the 
total  amount  found  in  the  organ  with  its  nerves  intact 
that  the  amount  normally  released  on  sympathetic 
nerve  stimulation  must  come  from  the  adrenergic 
nerves.  Analysis  of  the  urine  from  adrenalectomized 
patients  has  also  shown  that  the  amount  of  epineph- 
rine is  exceedingly  small  compared  with  that  of 
norepinephrine  (129).  Moreover,  no  increase  in  the 
epinephrine  output  was  observ-ed  in  the  adrenalecto- 
mized patients  subjected  to  tilting  head-up  which 
doubled  the  norepinephrine  output.  This  speaks 
strongly  against  the  assumption  that  epinephrine  is 
released  from  adrenergic  nerves  in  man.  Moreover, 
the  epinephrine  content  of  splenic  nerves  is  as  a  rule 
extremely  low,  a  fact  .suggesting  that  the  small  epi- 
nephrine amounts  found  in  spleen  extracts  (129)  or 
sometimes  in  the  effluent  blood  from  the  spleen  after 
stimulation  of  its  nerves  (108)  is  not  part  of  the 
neurotransmitter.  For  a  discussion  of  the  adrenoxine 
theory  of  Bacq  &  Heirman  (5)  the  reader  is  referred 
to  the  survey  on  this  subject  by  the  same  authors. 

It  is  clearly  a  matter  of  choice  whether  the  epi- 
nephrine released  from  chromaffin  cells  in  the  tissue 
upon  sympathetic  nerve  stimulation  should  be  re- 
garded as  a  chemical  transmitter.  If  one  agrees  to 
that  terminology,  the  release  of  suprarenal  medullary 
hormones  should  likewise  be  called  chemical  trans- 
mission. This,  however,  is  apt  to  cause  confusion  of 
the  concepts.  It  must  still  be  left  an  open  question 
whether  the  epinephrine-like  actions  observed  upon 


stimulation  of  sympathetic  fibers  to  the  skin  (57)  are 
due  to  a  release  from  chromaffin  cells. 

The  possibility  of  dopamine  serving  as  a  neurotrans- 
mitter requires  further  study.  Holtz,  Credner  & 
Koepp  (66)  showed  that  it  occurred  normallv  in 
urine.  Its  formation  was  explained  as  an  action  of 
/-dopadecarboxylase  on  dopa.  Later  dopamine  was 
demonstrated  by  Goodall  (55)  in  extracts  of  the 
suprarenal  gland  and  in  extracts  of  mammalian  heart. 

.Since  the  presence  of  catechol  amines  in  organs  is 
correlated  with  their  adrenergic  nerves  or  chromaffin 
cells,  it  might  be  expected  that  the  former  aLso  con- 
tain dopamine.  This  has  been  shown  to  be  the  case; 
dopamine  was  found  in  comparatively  large  amounts 
in  extracts  of  splenic  nerves  (i  17).  It  seems  reasonable 
to  assume  that  the  dopamine  found  in  organs  is  pres- 
ent in  their  adrenergic  nerves.  If  this  assumption  is 
correct  the  question  arises  as  to  how  dopamine  is 
stored  and  whether  it  is  released  upon  nerve  stimula- 
tion. Generally  the  amount  of  dopamine  in  an  organ 
is  hardly  large  enough  to  cause  biological  efifects 
comparable  to  those  caused  by  the  norepinephrine. 
However,  the  bovine  lung  contains  large  amounts  of 
dopamine  in  comparison  with  norepinephrine  (132), 
and  it  cannot  be  ruled  out  that  dopamine  exerts  bio- 
logical actions  in  this  case.  After  chromatographic 
separation  the  amount  of  dopamine  was  found  by  bio- 
logical and  chemical  methods  to  be  0.5  to  i  fig  per 
gm  tis.sue  while  the  norepinephrine  was  o.oi  to  0.03 
Mg  per  gm.  Since  the  biological  activity  of  the  two 
substances  is  approximately  in  the  proportion  50  to 
100: 1,  it  is  obvious  that  dopamine  may  be  biologicallv 
significant  in  the  lung. 

It  has  been  claimed  that  isopropylnorepinephrine 
occurs  in  small  amounts  in  extracts  of  the  adrenal 
gland  (81).  Apparently  the  amounts  are  too  small  to 
permit  detection  with  the  usual  colorimetric  and  bio- 
logical methods,  since  these  give  very  good  agree- 
ment with  the  figures  for  epinephrine  and  norepi- 
nephrine. However,  it  has  been  reported  that  it  can 
be  separated  by  chromatographic  technic,  a  certain 
fraction  showing  the  characteristic  biological  action 
of  the  isopropyl  compound. 

It  has  been  reported  that,  after  stimulation  of  the 
sympathetic  nerves  to  the  lungs,  the  isopropyl  com- 
pound appears  in  the  effluent  blood  (82).  Chromato- 
graphic separation  of  catechol  compounds  in  extracts 
of  up  to  1000  gm  bovine  lungs  have  failed  to  detect 
this  fraction,  although  catechol  acetic  acid,  dopamine 
and  norepinephrine  are  readily  identified  (132). 


23° 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


THE     CHOLINERGIC    NERVE     TRANSMITTER 

Identification 

Dixon  &  Hamill  (36)  pointed  out  as  early  as  1909 
that  there  was  very  little  inherent  difference  between 
the  action  of  muscarine  on  the  heart  and  electrical 
excitation  of  the  vagus.  They  continue:  "If  it  is  per- 
missible to  argue  from  analogy  there  is  reason  in  the 
suggestion  that  excitation  of  a  nerve  induces  the  local 
liberation  of  a  hormone  which  causes  specific  activity 
by  combination  with  some  constituent  by  the  end 
organ  muscle  or  gland."  Only  a  few  years  later 
Dale  (25)  and  Dale  &  Ewins  (30)  related  the  phe- 
nomena observed  on  stimulation  of  parasympathetic 
nerves  to  some  earlier  research  by  Hunt  &  Taveau 
(71).  Among  a  large  series  of  choline  esters  prepared 
by  them,  acetylcholine  was  shown  to  be  the  most  bio- 
logically active,  on  an  average  about  1000  times  more 
active  than  choline.  During  studies  on  ergot  extracts. 
Dale  (26)  found  a  substance  which  produced  actions 
similar  to  muscarine  and  identified  this  substance 
with  acetylcholine.  In  his  further  workJDale  was  able 
to  state  that  the  actions  of  vagus  stimulation  and  also 
other  actions  of  the  cranial  and  sacral  divisions  of  the 
autonomic  system  were  mimicked  very  faithfully  by 
ac_etylcholine.  The  effects  were  remarkably  evanescent 
and  were  always  abolished  by  a  small  dose  of  atro- 
pine. On  the  basis  of  these  observations  by  Dale  it 
became  extremely  likely  that  acetylcholine  actually 
was  the  substance  which  causes  the  effect  of  para- 
sympathetic nerve  impulses  on  the  target  cells. 
Further  support  for  the  idea  that  the  substance  re- 
leased at  the  parasympathetic  nerve  endings  was 
acetylcholine  was  supplied  by  Dale  &  Dudley  (29) 
who  showed  in  1929  that  it  was  present  in  the  spleen 
of  the  horse  and  the  ox.  They  prepared  the  substance 
and  isolated  it  as  chloroplatinate. 

The  identification  of  the  parasympathetic  cholin- 
ergic nerve  transmitter  is  based  upon  biological  tests. 
The  amounts  of  acetylcholine  which  are  liberated 
and  occur  in  the  organism  are  generally  too  small  to 
be  determined  by  chemical  methods.  Some  of  the  bio- 
logical methods  are  very  sensitive,  but  on  the  other 
hand  the  specificity  is  not  always  above  doubt.  The 
methods  most  widely  used  are  the  negative  inotropic 
action  of  acetylcholine  on  the  heart  of  the  frog,  the 
hypotensive  effect  in  the  cat  and  the  contracting;  effect 
on  the  intestine  of  the  guinea  pig  or  other  animals. 
Other  preparations  which  may  yield  more  specific 
results  are  the  leech  muscle,  the  rectus  abdominis 
muscle  and  the  isolated  lung  of  the  frog.  The  isolated 


heart  of  the  clam  Venus  mercenaria  has  also  been  used. 
For  the  identification  of  acetylcholine,  the  finding  of 
Fiihner  (49)  that  the  dorsal  muscle  of  the  leech  was 
greatly  .sensitized  to  acetylcholine  by  addition  of 
physostigmine  was  one  of  the  more  important.  The 
preparation  was  introduced  as  a  specific  and  quanti- 
tative biologic  test  for  acetylcholine  in  1932  by  Minz 
(95).  After  preparation  the  muscle  is  suspended  in 
Ringer's  solution  from  one-half  to  several  hours  to 
relax  it,  and  physostigmine  is  added  to  the  solution  in 
a  concentration  of  1-200,000  to  1-2,000,000.  After 
about  20  min.,  the  muscle  is  highly  sensitized  to 
acetylcholine  so  as  to  detect  and  measure  acetylcho- 
line in  concentrations  as  low  as  io~'.  The  frog  rectus 
is  less  .sensitive  but  fairly  specific  for  acetylcholine. 
The  isolated  frog  lung  has  also  been  used  and  may 
have  an  even  higher  sensitivity  than  the  leech  muscle; 
it  is  claimed  to  contract  in  an  acetylcholine  solution 
of  io~'*  (34)-  The  heart  of  Venus  mercenaria  has  also 
been  reported  to  have  high  sensitivity  to  acetylcholine, 
up  to  io~^-,  although  it  varies  at  different  times  of  the 
year. 

In  order  to  allow  the  conclusion  that  the  actions 
observed  on  these  test  preparations  actually  have 
been  due  to  acetylcholine,  certain  other  conditions 
must  be  fulfilled.  The  action  has  to  be  increased  by 
drugs  inhibiting  the  acetylcholine  esterase  such  as_ 
physostigmine,  the  activity  should  disappear  after 
incubation  with  blood  and  the  active  principle  should 
be  inactivated  when  exposed  to  in  alkali  for  10  min. 
at  room  temperature,  which  is  typical  of  choline 
esters.  As  a  general  rule  different  kinds  of  tests  have 
to  be  consistent,  i.e.  when  compared  with  a  standard 
of  acetylcholine  the  unknown  extracts  should  elicit 
the  same  quantitative  action  in  relation  to  acetyl- 
choline (fig.  12). 

One  of  the  chief  difficulties  in  demonstrating  the 
neurochemical  transmission  from  cholinergic  nerves 
arises  from  the  fact  that  in  most  cases  the  para- 
sympathetic nerves  have  their  autonomic  synapses 
very  close  to  the  target  organ.  Therefore,  stimulation 
of  the  nerves  also  releases  acetylcholine  from  the 
preganglionic  nerve.  The  acetylcholine  released  by 
stimulation  of  vagus  nerve  in  the  frog's  heart  may 
actually  be  due  partly  to  the  release  of  the  substance 
from  the  synapses. 

The  introduction  of  physostigmine  in  experimental 
work  made  it  possible  to  demonstrate  the  mediated 
effect  with  greater  certainty  since  the  substance  was 
not  immediately  destroyed.  Loewi's  original  experi- 
ments were  later  confirmed  by  many  others.  Among 
the  sources  of  transmitter  which  have  been  tried  mav 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION  23 1 


FIG.  12.  Tests  of  a  perfusate  of  physostigminized  Locke's  solution  passing  through  the  vessels  of 
the  stomach  of  a  dog  during  vagal  stimulation.  The  samples  collected  before  stimulation  were  slightly, 
if  at  all,  active.  A:  effects  on  the  arterial  pressure  of  a  physostigminized  cat  under  chloralose.  B: 
isolated  frog  heart  (Straub).  C:  physostigminized  rectus  abdominis  of  the  frog.  D :  physostigminized 
leech  muscle.  In  each  series,  B  shows  the  effect  of  the  perfusate  collected  during  vagal  stimulation ; 
A  and  C  correspond  to  two  strengths  of  acetylcholine  (C  is  double  A').  [From  Dale  &  Feldberg 
(300 


be  mentioned  the  heart  and  the  sahvary  glands 
(61,  134)  in  physostigminized  animals.  Particularly 
illuminating  were  the  experiments  by  Feldberg  & 
Krayer  (44)  who  showed  that  blood  from  the  coronary 
veins  of  physostigminized  animals  produced  a  con- 
traction of  the  leech  muscle  shortly  after  vagal  stimu- 
lation. This  effect  was  abolished  by  atropine  and  the 
active  substance  was  destroyed  by  blood.  Even  re- 
flexly  released  transmitter  was  demonstrated  in  this 
way. 

Although  the  cholinergic  transmitter  has  not  been 
identified  with  the  same  certainty  as  the  adrenergic 
one,  the  sum  of  evidence  obtained  by  indirect  methods 
leaves  no  serious  doubt  that  it  is  either  acetylcholine 
or  some  other  choline  ester  with  very  similar  action 

(15)- 

In  the  autonomic  neurotransmission  to  the  salivary 
glands  both  adrenergic  and  cholinergic  fibers  seem  to 
take  part.  By  studying  the  distribution  of  cholin- 
esterase  Koelle  (75)  found  in  the  cat,  rabbit  and 
rhesus  monkey  that  the  concentration  of  the  true 
cholinesterase  was  higher  in  cholinergic  neurons 
than    in    adrenergic    and    sensory    neurons.    Cholin- 


esterase was  also  found  to  form  a  fine  network  around 
the  outside  of  the  acini  while  it  was  not  found  in  the 
acinar  cells  (118).  The  network  is  united  with  the 
nerve  trunk  and  is  considered  to  be  cholinergic  in  the 
submaxillary  gland  and  adrenergic  in  the  sublingual 
gland. 

Occurrence,  Biosynthesis  and  Storage 

It  may  be  assumed  that,  if  the  postganglionic  nerve 
endings  release  acetylcholine  during  nerve  stimula- 
tion, this  has  been  synthesized  and  stored  in  the  axon. 
For  this  reason  acetylcholine  would  be  expected  to 
occur  as  a  natural  constituent  of  cholinergic  nerves. 
In  this  connection  only  the  postganglionic  fibers  are 
being  considered.  Analysis  of  the  acetylcholine  con- 
tent of  such  fibers  has  shown  large  amounts  in  the 
short  ciliary  nerves,  3  to  8  /ng  per  gm,  which  is  only 
a  little  less  than  the  amounts  found  in  motor  nerve 
fibers  or  in  preganglionic  fibers  (92,  125).  The  figures 
are  much  higher  than  the  acetylcholine  content  in 
postganglionic  sympathetic  fibers,  such  as  the  splenic 
nerve  where  the  acetvlcholine-like  action  onlv  corre- 


232 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


sponds  to  0.2  to  0.5  Mg  per  gni-  The  difFercnce  in 
content  suggests  a  specific  function  of  the  excess 
acetylcholine  in  the  postganglionic  parasympathetic 
fibers.  The  high  content  in  these  may  be  regarded  as 
strong  support  of  the  idea  that  these  fibers  act  by 
releasing  acetylcholine.  [For  further  details  concern- 
ing the  occurrence  and  biosynthesis  of  acetylcholine 
in  cholinergic  nerves  see  Burgen  &  Macintosh  (15), 
Gaddum  (50)  and  Rosenblueth  (113).] 

The  method  of  estimating  the  amount  of  acetylcho- 
line directly  in  the  tissue  cannot  be  used,  however,  to 
estimate  the  amount  of  the  cholinergic  postganglionic 
transmitter,  since  this  will  also  occur  in  preganglionic 
autonomic  fibers  and  in  motor  nerves  and  possibly 
also  in  small  amounts  in  all  kinds  of  nerves. 

The  biosynthesis  of  the  cholinergic  transmitter 
has  been  l.ir^c  K  elucidated  by  the  studies  of 
Nachmansohn  &  Machado  (102).  These  authors 
were  ai^le  to  show  that  an  extract  from  rat  brain  con- 
tained an  enzyme  system  which  could  synthesize 
acetylcholine  in  the  presence  of  ATP  as  the  source  of 
energy.  This  enzyme  was  called  choline  aceL)dase.  It 
was  shown  later  that  the  acetylcholine  synthesis  occurs 
in  two  steps.  In  a  first  reaction  acetate  is  transformed 
into  active  acetate,  and  in  a  second  the  active  acetate 
combines  with  choline  to  form  acetylcholine  (78). 
The  research  work  of  Stern  &  Ochoa  ( 1 20)  and  others 
indicates  that  choline  acetylase  catalyzes  the  last  step 
in  the  acetylcholine  formation  and  that  the  aciise 
acetate  is  an  acetyl  coenzyme  (coenzyme  A).  The 
acetate  used  for  the  synthesis  has  to  be  activated  by 
inc. Ills  of  ATP,  coenzyme  and  a  transacetylase.  The 
active  acetate  thus  formed  is  used  for  the  final  synthe- 
sis of  the  acetylcholine.  Choline  acetylase  has  been 
extracted  from  brain  and  from  electric  organs  but  is 
also  present  in  all  nerve  tissues.  It  has  even  been 
demonstrated  in  tissues  from  various  invertebrates, 
such  as  annelids  and  flatworms.  The  presence  of  cho- 
line acetylase  in  mitochondrial  fractions  in  homoge- 
nates  of  brain  (60)  suggests  that  this  may  be  the  case 
al.so  in  the  postganglionic  neurons. 

As  to  the  storage  of  the  cholinergic  transmitter  it 
appears  likely  that  it  is  confined  to  structural  elements 
as  demonstrated  for  adrenergic  nerves.  Some  indirect 
support  for  the  opinion  that  acetylcholine  is  also  in- 
closed in  separate  particles  may  be  found  in  the  early 
experiment  by  Loewi  &  Hellauer  (86).  Loewi  (85) 
points  to  the  finding  that  when  nerve  tissue  is  ex- 
tracted with  Ringer's  solution,  the  bulk  of  acetylcho- 
line is  found  in  the  insoluble  residue  but  that  the  use 
of  hypotonic  .solution  causes  the  greater  part  of  the 
acetylcholine  to  be  released.  This  suggests  that  the 


acetylcholine  is  located  in  particles  surrounded  ijy  a 
membrane  similar  to  mitochondria.  When  Ringer's 
solution  is  used  for  extracting  the  acetylcholine  in  a 
cholinergic  nerve,  such  as  the  vagus,  most  of  the 
acetylcholine  goes  into  solution,  however.  It  is  also 
noteworthy  that  when  acidified  solutions  are  used, 
the  total  amount  of  acetylcholine  is  extracted  as  is 
also  the  case  when  extraction  is  made  with  acidified 
alcohol.  Some  of  the  acetylcholine  may  be  i^ound  to 
some  lipid  complex  soluble  in  ether,  which  acetylcho- 
line in  itself  is  not  (86). 

An  analogous  i^ehavior  is  shown  by  epinephrine 
and  norepinephrine  and  histamine.  It  therefore  seems 
possible  that  these  amines  form  ether-soluble  but 
water-insoluble  compounds  in  the  particles.  It  is  of 
interest  in  this  connection  that  Hillarp  &  Nilson  (64) 
found  a  high  content  of  phosphatides  in  the  supra- 
renal medullary  granules. 

Release  in  Organs 

Very  little  is  known  concerning  the  mechanism  of 
release  of  the  cholinergic  transmitter  in  the  autonomic 
neuromuscular  junctions.  By  studying  the  release  of 
acetylcholine  from  the  spontaneously  beating  or  elec- 
trically driven  rabbit's  heart,  it  has  been  possible  to 
show  that  the  release  is  significantly  higher  at  a 
faster  heart  rate.  Thus  a  spontaneously  beating  heart 
with  a  mean  rate  of  56  per  min.  released  0.26  ± 
0.08  ^g  per  heart  in  40  min.  while  electrically  driven 
hearts  with  a  mean  rate  of  210  per  min.  released 
0.97  ±  0.36  Kg  per  heart  in  40  min.  (32). 

The  relea.se  of  acetylcholine  from  an  organ  does 
not  necessarily  mean  that  this  substance  originates 
from  nervous  tissue  since  it  is  known  that  even  nerve- 
free  tissue  is  able  to  synthesize  and  release  acetylcho- 
line (17).  '^" 

Most  of  the  knowledge  on  the  action  of  acet\  Icho- 
line  and  its  release  refers  to  the  motor  endplate  which 
has  been  studied  in  detail  from  a  chemical  point  of 
view  as  well  as  by  electrophysiological  techniques. 
There  is  hardly  any  douijt,  however,  that  the  mecha- 
nism of  relea.se  of  the  cholinergic  transmitter  from  the 
postganglionic  cholinergic  nerves  is  similar  in  kind  to 
that  already  outlined  for  the  adrenergic  transmitter. 
We  may  thus  assume  that  the  active  transmitter  is 
released  at  a  terminal  portion  of  the  ner\e  and  acts 
directly  in  a  chemical  manner  on  the  smooth  muscle 
fibers.  There  is  no  reason  to  believe  that  the  sijiooth 
muscle  cells  are  directly  innervated  by  cholinergic 
postganglionic  fibers  any  more  than  they  are  by 
adrenergic  fibers. 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


•^33 


RELEASE  FROM  ISOLATED  NERVES.  Although  no  experi- 
ments seem  to  have  been  made  with  stimulation  of 
postgansrlionic  cholinergic  nerves,  several  authors 
have  reported  that  stimulation  of  cholinergic  pre- 
ganglionic nerves  causes  a  release  of  acetylcholine 
(2,  10,  23,  79).  It  may  be  assumed  that  similar  events 
take  place  during  stimulation  of  postganglionic 
cholinergic  nerves. 

The  inhibitory  action  of  atropine  on  the  effect  of 
cholinergic  nerve  stimulation  has  been  shown  to 
depend  on  blocking  of  the  target  cell  to  the  released 
transmitter.  It  was  demonstrated  by  Feldberg  & 
Krayer  (44)  that  atropine  does  riotjnterfere  with  the 
release  as  such. 

The  failure  of  atropine  to  block  the  effect  of  stimu- 
lating the  vagus  nerve  on  the  intestine  may  be  due  to 
an  action  of  a  transmitter  different  from  acetylcholine, 
released  from  the  enteric  nerve  system.  The  nature  of 
this  postulated  transmitter  is  not  known,  but  it  should 
be  recalled  that  substance  P  (51}  occurs  in  the  in- 
testine and  is  insensitive  to  atropine. 

Rosenblueth  (113)  has  advanced  the  idea  that  the 
cholinergic  nerve  transmission  proceeds  in  two  stages 
of  which  the  first  is  a  release  of  acetylcholine  followed 
ijy  a  .second  in  which  the  nerve  transmitter  subse- 
ciucntly  forms  '  paras)mpathin'  which  then  acts 
directly  on  the  target  cell. 

Stimulation  experiments  on  postganglionic  cho- 
linergic nerves  (short  ciliary  nerves)  have  shown  that 
the  optimum  frequency  is  about  25  per  sec.  (89).  As 
in  the  case  of  adrenergic  nerves,  prolonged  stimula- 
tion caused  Qnlj'  slight  signs  of  exhaustibility.  Thus 
stimulation  for  i  to  2  hours  caused  a  sustained  con- 
traction of  the  iris;  thereafter  the  effect  gradually 
declined. 

Removal  of  Trorumitter 

As  early  as  191 4  Dale  (25)  had  assumed  that 
acetylcholine  was  destroyed  rapidly  in  the  organism 
by  some  hydrolyzing  enzyme.  Such  an  enzyme  was 
actually  discovered  by  Loewi  &  Navratil  (87)  in  ex- 
tracts of  frog's  heart.  They  also  found  that  this  .en^ 
zyme  could  i)c  inhibited  by  physostiginine.  This  was 
in  agreement  with  the  results  of  earlier  experiinents 
of  Dixon  &  Brodie  (35)  and  others  who  found  that 
this  drug  increased  some  of  the  effects  of  parasympa- 
thetic nerve  stimulation.  Moreover,  Loewi  &  Navratil 
were  able  to  show  that  it  increased  the  effect  of  the 
substance  liberated  from  the  frog's  heart  upon  stimu- 
lation. The  '  Vagusstoff '  thus  behaved  like  a  choline 
ester  since  it  was  a)  inhibited  by  atropine  which  is  a 


specific  inhibitor,  at  least  in  small  doses,  and  A)  pro- 
tected by  physostigmine  which  is  known  to  inhibit 
choline  esterase.  It  is  generally  assumed  that  the 
cholinergic  transmitter  is  being  inactivated  locally  to 
a  great  extent.  Information  about  the  distribution  of 
cholinesterase  in  the  peripheral  tissue  is  accumulating 
rapidly  as  a  result  of  the  development  of  suitable 
methods.  This  includes  important  findings  about  the 
distribution  of  cholinesterase  at  the  motor  endplates 
and  in  the  central  nervous  system  (43,  77).  It  may  be 
assumed  that  part  of  the  transmitter  released  in 
peripheral  organs,  such  as  the  smooth  inuscle  organs 
and  glands,  is  diffusing  out  in  the  blood  stream  where 
it  is  rapidly  inactivated  by  the  cholinesterase  present. 
It  is  also  possible  that  cholinesterase  is  present  in  the 
target  cells  in  amounts  large  enough  to  destroy  any 
ainount  of  the  transmitter  diffusing  into  the  cell. 


MECHANISM   OF   ACTION   OF   NEUROTRANSMITTERS 

The  neurotransmitters  exert  direct  action  on  target 
cells  independently  of  whether  or  not  the  cells  are 
autonomically  innervated.  This  is  shown  by  the  pro- 
nounced action  of  the  transmitter  substances  on  nerve- 
free  organs,  like  the  placenta,  or  on  denervated  struc- 
tures. 

The  mode  of  action  of  the  neurotransmitters  on  the 
target  cells  has  been  much  discussed.  Clark  (24) 
related  the  minimal  effective  doses  of  acetylcholine 
and  epinephrine  on  the  frog's  heart  and  the  frog's 
stomach  to  the  total  surface  of  the  cells  affected  and 
arrived  at  the  conclusion  that  while  the  effective  dose 
of  0.02  /tig  per  gm  covered  a  surface  of  al)out  i  cm-  the 
total  area  of  the  cells  was  6000  to  20000  cm-.  For  this 
reason  it  was  obvious  that  the  aQjive  suijstance  only 
needed  to  attack  a  minute  part  of  the  cell  in  order  to 
elicit  itijction. 

It  is  generally  a.ssumed  that  the  active  substance, 
be  it  a  neurotransmitter  or  a  pharmacologically 
active  drug  of  a  different  kind,  has  to  unite  in  some 
way  with  the  target  cell  before  e.xerting  its  action. 
Often  the  sites  of  binding  between  the  cell  and  the 
active  molecule  are  referred  to  as  receptors.  According 
to  Clark  these  postulated  receptors,  in  or  on  the  cell, 
occupy  only  a  very  small  portion  of  the  cell  volume 
or  surface.  Morphological  evidence  for  specific  re- 
ceptor patches  on  the  cell  surface  is  still  lacking, 
however. 

A  discussion  of  the  number  of  inolecules  of  a  trans- 
mitter required  to  activate  a  single  cell  depends  obvi- 
ously on  the  type  of  administration  and  on  the  sensi- 


234 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tivitv  of  the  cell.  If  0.02  fig  acetylcholine  is  necessary 
to  inhibit  one  gram  of  frog's  heart,  as  in  Clark's  ex- 
ample, the  minimal  effective  amount  per  gm  of  tissue 
is  about  10"  molecules  per  cell.  In  isolated  organs 
entirely  different  results  may  be  obtained.  Thus 
0.05  mjug  epinephrine  per  ml  suspension  fluid  is  some- 
times enough  to  elicit  an  inhibitory  effect  on  the 
fowl's  isolated  rectal  cecum.  In  this  case  obviously  a 
much  smaller  numijcr  of  molecules  are  capable  of 
producing  the  action,  since  most  of  them  are  in  the 
suspension  fluid  without  contact  with  the  organ.  If  it 
is  assumed  that  i  o  per  cent  of  the  molecules  are  acting 
on  I  gm  of  organ  containing  10'"  cells,  then  the  num- 
ber of  molecules  per  cell  will  be  only  10,  provided 
that  the  active  substance  is  distributed  on  all  cells. 
This  is  probably  not  the  case.  All  calculations  of  this 
kind  therefore  appear  very  dubious. 

It  is  conceivable  that  the  neurotransmitter  takes 
part  in  a  chemical  reaction  sequence  which  is  influ- 
enced thereby  in  a  quantitative  or  even  qualitative 
manner.  Whether  this  action  is  initiated  at  specific 
receptor  patches  at  the  surface  or  at  specific  metabolic 
structure  elements  in  the  interior  of  the  cell  is  not 
known.  It  may  be  recalled  that  there  is  good  evidence 
for  the  permeation  of  neurotransmitters  through  cell 
membranes,  since  this  is  the  basis  for  most  of  our 
information  regarding  their  release. 

Elaborate  schemes  of  receptor  mechanisms  have 
been  presented  by  several  authors  and  terms  sug- 
gested for  the  postulated  receptors.  Since  these  efforts 
primarilv  represent  an  attempt  to  put  the  known  facts 
in  a  formal  system  but  hardly  contribute  to  our  actual 
knowledge,  these  systems  will  not  be  dealt  with  here. 
Recent  contributions  to  the  discussion  have  been 
given  by  Zupancic  (137)  and  Stephenson  (iig). 

How  the  neurotransmitter  elicits  a  relaxation  or  a 
contraction  of  the  target  cell  is  still  ob.scure.  It  can  be 
assumed  that  the  active  substance  initiates  or  rein- 
forces soine  process  which  eventually  causes  physico- 
chemical  changes  in  the  contractile  material  con- 
ducive to  such  effects. 

Attempts  have  been  made  to  correlate  the  inhibi- 
tory actions  of  epinephrine  with  the  formation  of 
lactic  acid  (99),  which  is  believed  to  be  the  metabolic 
product  directly  responsible  for  the  inhiljitory  action. 
The  hypothesis  obviously  requires  that  the  widely 
varying  activity  ratios  of  epinephrine  and  norepi- 
nephrine for  an  organ  like  the  fowl's  rectal  cecum 
(from  4  to  200)  are  associated  with  corresponding 
variations  in  the  formation  of  lactic  acid  in  the  react- 
ing target  cells,  a  demonstration  which  has  not  been 
made 


On  account  of  the  large  differences  in  action  be- 
tween the  levo-  and  dextroisomers  of  epinephrine, 
for  instance,  it  has  been  inferred  that  the  active  sub- 
stance combines  with  an  optically  active  constituent 
of  the  cell  (122).  Recent  careful  studies  on  the  bio- 
logical activity  of  optical  isomers  of  sympathomimetic 
amines  have  shown  that  the  difference  in  action  be- 
tween the  isomers  is  even  greater  than  has  been 
hitherto  recognized  (90). 

These  results  suggest  that  the  neurotransmitter  is 
involved  in  enzymatic  reactions,  a  conclusion  which 
also  appears  most  likely  for  other  reasons. 

Another  approach  to  the  study  of  the  mode  of 
action  of  neurotransmitters  on  the  target  cell  is  based 
on  the  quantitative  relationships  between  do.se  and 
action.  Such  quantitation  of  the  effects  has  ijeen  used 
for  the  elaboration  of  formulae  of  \arious  kinds.  It  is 
outside  the  scope  of  this  article  to  discuss  these  studies. 
It  may  be  said  generally,  however,  that  by  applying 
this  principle  to  single  cells  more  information  may  be 
gained.  In  most  cases  the  relationship  between  dose 
and  action  is  approximately  expressed  by  a  rectangu- 
lar hyperbola.  Its  precise  biological  significance  is 
not  as  yet  clear. 

Summarizing,  it  may  be  concluded  that  not  much 
more  knowledge  aijout  the  mode  of  action  of  the 
neurotransmitters  on  target  cells  has  been  gained 
since  Langley's  time  when  he  ascribed  the  differenti- 
ating effect  of  the  transmitter,  relaxation  or  contrac- 
tion, to  a  receptor  substance  in  the  cell. 

A  relevant  question  is  whether  two  neurotrans- 
mitters released  in  the  same  organ  act  on  the  same  or 
different  cells  and  to  what  extent  they  interfere  with 
one  another's  actions.  Morison  &  Acheson  (20)  found 
similar  hyperijolic  concentration-action  curves  for 
epinephrine  and  acetylcholine  on  the  nictitating  mem- 
brane of  the  cat.  When  the  two  substances  were  in- 
jected together,  their  actions  added  up  along  the  same 
curve.  These  results  would  seem  to  allow  the  impor- 
tant conclusion  that  the  two  neurotransmitters  act 
independently  by  exerting  separate  actions.  Whether 
or  not  these  are  on  the  same  or  different  cells  cannot 
be  decided  from  these  experiments. 


NEUROTRANSMITTERS   IN    BLOOD    AND    URINE 

It  has  been  discussed  above  that  some  of  the  neuro- 
transmitter released  at  the  autonomic  postganglionic 
nerve  endings  passes  bevond  the  target  cells  and 
reaches  the  l)lood  stream.  If  this  occurs  to  any  con- 
siderable extent  it  should  be  possible  to  demonstrate 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


235 


the  neurotransmitters  in  the  blood.  Such  attempts 
have  been  made  and  there  is  some  evidence  for  the 
opinion  that  the  neurotransmitter  of  the  adrenergic 
system  normally  occurs  in  small  quantities  in  the 
blood.  However,  since  the  methods  of  demonstrating 
norepinephrine  in  blood  require  fairly  large  quanti- 
ties of  blood  and  are  rather  laijorious  (70,  91),  they 
have  not  been  widely  used.  Some  indirect  informa- 
tion has  been  obtained  by  studying  the  excretion  in 
urine  (67,  130).  Even  if  norepinephrine  occurs  in 
peripheral  blood  it  remains  to  be  shown  that  it  is 
derived  from  postganglionic  nerves  and  not  from  the 


suprarenal  medulla  or  from  chromafhn  cells.  Proof  of 
its  overflow  and  passage  into  the  blood  has  been  given, 
however,  by  studies  on  the  excretion  in  urine  in 
adrenalectomized  patients.  In  these  patients  the  only 
important  sources  of  norepinephrine  can  be  the  post- 
ganglionic nerves.  For  the  same  rea.sons  as  outlined 
above  the  excretion  of  acetylcholine,  after  treatment 
of  the  organism  with  physostigmine,  will  not  allow 
any  conclusions  as  to  the  release  at  the  postganglionic 
nerve  endings  since  acetylcholine  is  also  released  at 
manv  other  sites. 


REFERENCES 


1.  Allen,   W.  J.,   H.   Barcroft  .\nd   O.   G.    Edholm.  J.  26. 
Physiol.  105:255,  1946.  27. 

2.  Babsky,  E.  B.  Bull.  Biol.  Med.  Exper.  USSR  5:  51,  1938.  28. 

3.  Bacq,  Z.  M.  Arch,  inlcnial.  phjsiol.  36:  167,  1933.  29. 

4.  Bacq,  Z.  M.  Ann.  Physiol.  10:  467,  1934. 

5.  Bacq,   Z.    M.    and    p.    Heirman.    Arch,    internal,  phy.siol.  30. 
50:  153,  1940.  31. 

6.  Bain,  W.  A.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  23:  381,  1933. 

7.  Bain,  W.  A.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  22:  269,  1932.  32. 

8.  Barcroft,  H.  and  H.  J.  C.  Swan.  Sympathetic  Control  of  33. 
Human  Blood  Vessels.  London :  Arnold,  1 953. 

9.  Barger,  G.  and  H.  H.  Dale.  J.  Physiol.  41 :  19,  1910-1 1.  34. 

10.  Berg.\mi,  G.  Boll.  Soc.  ital.  biol.  sper.  11:  275,  1936. 

1 1.  Blaschko,  H.  J.  Physiol.  loi :  337,  1942.  35. 

12.  Blaschko,   H.   and  A.   D.   Welch.   Arch,   exper.   Path.   u. 
Pharmakol.  219:  17,  1953.  36. 

13.  Brinkman,  R.   and  E.  van  Dam.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.    196;  37. 
66,  1922. 

14.  Brown,  G.  L.  and  J.  S.  Gillespie.  .Vature,  London.    178:  38. 
980,  1956.  39. 

15.  Burden,   A.   S.   V.   and   F.    C.   Macintosh.    In:  .Veuro-  40. 
chemistry,  edited  by  K.  A.  C.  Elliott,  I.  H.  Page  and  J.  H. 
Quastcl.  Springfield:  Thomas,  1955,  p.  311.  41. 

16.  Burn,  J.  H.  and  J.  Robinson.  Brit.  J.  Pharmacol.  6:  loi,  42. 

■95>-  43- 

17.  Burn,  J.    H.,    E.    M.    Vaughan   Williams   and  J.    M.  44. 
Walker.  J.  Physiol.   131;  317,  1956. 

18.  Cannon,  W.  B.  and  K.  Lissak.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   125:  765,  45. 

1939-  46- 

19.  Cannon,   W.   B.    and   A.   Rosenblueth.   Am.  J.    Physiol.  47. 

'04:5.57.  '933-  48- 

20.  Cannon,  W.   B.   and  A.   Rosenblueth.  Autonomic  .\'euro- 
Effector  Systems.  New  York:  Macmillan,  1937.  49. 

21.  Cannon,   W.   B.    and   J.    E.   Uridil.   Am.  J.   Physiol.   58:  50. 
353.  1921- 

22.  Celander,  O.    Acta  physiol.    scandinav.  suppl.    116,   32:    i,  51. 

1954- 

23.  Chang,  H.  C,  W.  M.  Hsieh,  T.  H.  Li  and  R.  K.  S.  52. 
LiM.  Chiruse  J.  Physiol.   14:  19,  1939. 

24.  Clark,  A.  J.  The  Mode  of  Action  of  Drugs  on  Cells.  London:  53. 
Arnold,  1933. 

25.  Dale,    H.    H.   J.    Pharmacol.   &    Exper.    Therap.    6:    147,  54. 
'9'4- 


Dale,  H.  H.  J.  Physiol.  48:  iii,  1914. 

Dale,  H.  H.  J.  Physiol.  80:  ioP,^i933. 

Dale,  H.  H.  Brit.  M.  J.  I:  835,  1934. 

Dale,   H.   H.   and   H.   W.    Dudley.   J.    Physiol.   68:  97, 

1929. 

Dale,  H.  H.  and  A.  J.  Ewins.  J.  Physiol.  48:  xxiv,  1914. 

Dale,    H.   H.    and   W.    Feldberg.   J.   Physiol.   81 :   320, 

1934- 
Day,  M.  J.  Physiol.  134:  558,  1956. 

Demis,  D.  J.,  H.  Blaschko  and  .\.  D.  Welch.  J.  Phar- 
macol. &  Exper.  Therab.  117:  208,  1956. 
Dijkstra,  C.  and  .\.  K.  M.  Novons.  Arch,  internal,  physiol. 
49:  257.  1939- 
Dixon,   W.    E.    and   T.   G.   Brodie.   J.    Physiol.,    29:   97, 

1903- 

Dixon,  W.  E.  and  P.  Hamill.  J.  Physiol.  38:  314,  1909. 

Duncanson,  D.,  T.  Stewart  and  O.  G.  Edholm.  Fed. 

Proc.  8:37,  1949. 

DuNER,  H.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  32:  63,  1954. 

D-t-E,  J.  \.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  113:  265,  1935. 

Eli.\sson,    R.,    U.    S.    v.    EuLER    AND   L.    Stjarne.    Acta 

physiol.  scandinav.  Suppl.  118,  33:  63,  1955. 

Elliott,  T.  R.  J.  Physiol.  32:  401,  1905. 

Emmelin,  N.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  34:  29,  1955. 

Feldberg,  W.  S.  Pharmacol.  Rev.  6:  85,  1954. 

Feldberg,    W.    and    O.    Krayer.    Arch,    exper.    Path.    u. 

Pharmakol.  172:  170,  1933. 

Finkleman,  B.  J.  Physiol.  70:  145,  1930. 

FoLKow,  B.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  25:  49,  1952. 

FoLKOw,  B.  Physiol.  Rev.  35:  629,  1955. 

FoLKOW,   B.    and   B.    Uvn.Ks.   Ada  physiol.   scandinav.    15: 

365,  1948. 

FiJHNER,  H.  Arch,  exper.  Path.  u.  Pharmakol.  82:  57,   1918. 

Gaddum,  J.  H.  Gefdsserweiternde  Stoffe  der  Gewebe.  Leipzig: 

Thieme,  1936. 

Gaddum,  J.  H.  Polypeptides  Which  .S'timulale  Plain  Muscle. 

Edinburgh:  Livingstone,  1955. 

Gaddum,  J.   H.    and   L.   G.   Goodwin.   J.    Physiol.    105: 

357.  >947- 

Gaddum,  J.    H.,    M.    A.    Khayyal   and   H.    Rydin.   J. 

Physiol.  89:9,  1937. 

Garven,  H.   S.  D.   and  F    W.   Gairns.  Quart.  J.  Exper. 

Physiol.  37:  131,  1952. 


236 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


55.  GooDALL,   McCh.   Ada  phrsiol.    scandinar.   24:    suppl.    85, 

I95I- 

56.  Greef,  K.  and  p.  Hoi.tz.  Aich.  inlntuil.  pfiarmmody?!.  88: 

228,  1951. 

57.  Green,  H.  D.,  J.  A.  MacLeod,  D.  A.  Anderson  and 
A.  B.  Denison,  Jr.  J.  Pharmacol.  &  Exper.  Therap.  112: 
218,  1954. 

58.  Greer,  C.  M.,  J.  O.  Pinkston,  J.  H.  Baxter  and  E.  S. 
Brannon.  J.  Pharmacol.  &  Exper.  Therap.  62:  189,  1938. 

59.  Hagen,  p.  J.  Pharmacol.  &  Exper.  Therap.  1 16:  26,  1956. 

60.  Hebb,  C.  O.  and  B.  N.  Smallman.  J.  Physiol.  134:  385, 
1956. 

61.  Henderson,  V.  E.  and  M.  H.  Roepke.  Arch,  exper.  Path, 
u.  Pharmakol.  172;  314,  1933. 

62.  HiLLARP,  N.-A.  Acta  anal.  1:  suppl.  4,  1946. 

63.  HiLLARP,  N.-A.  AND  B.  NiLSON.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav. 
31:  suppl.  113^  79,  1954. 

64.  HiLLARP,  N.-A.  AND  B.  NiLSON.  Acta  pliyswl.  scandinav.  32: 

II,  1954- 

65.  Hoffmann,  F.,  E.  J.  Hoffmann,  .S.  Middleton  and  J. 
Talesnik.  Am.  J.kPhvsiol.  144:  i8g,  1945. 

66.  Holtz,  p.,  K.  Credner  and  W.  Koepp.  .4rch.  exper.  Path, 
u.  Pharmakol.  200:  356,  1942. 

67.  Holtz,  P.,  K.  Credner  and  G.  Kroneberg.  Arch, 
exper.  Path.  u.  Pharmakol.  204;  228,  1947. 

68.  Holtz,  P.,  R.  Heise  and  K.  Ludtke.  Arch,  exper.  Path.  u. 
Pharmakol.  191:  87,  1938-39. 

69.  Holtz,  P.  and  E.  Westermann.  Arch,  exper.  Path.  u. 
Pharmakol.  227:  538,  1956. 

70.  Holzbauer,    M.    and   M.   Vogt.    Brit.   J.    Pharmakol.    g: 

•^49.  '954- 

71.  Hunt,  R.  and  R.  de  M.  Taveau.  Brit.  M.  J.  2;   1788, 

1906. 

72.  Jabonero,  v.  Acta  neuroveg.  Suppl.  6,  1955. 

73.  James,  W.  O.  .Nature,  London  161  :  851,   1948. 

74.  Kirshner,  N.  and  McCh.  Goodall.  Fed.  Proc.    15:    iio, 

1956. 

75.  KOELLE,  G.   B.  J.   Pharmacol.  G?  Exper.    Therap.    114:    167, 

1955- 

76.  KoELLE,  G.  B.  AND  A.  DE  T.  Valk,  Jr.  J.  Physiol.    126: 

434.  1954- 

77.  KuFFLER,  S.  VV.  .irch.  sc.  physiol.  3:  585,  1949. 

78.  Lipton,  M.  a.  and  E.  S.  G.  Barron.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  166: 
367,  1946. 

79.  L1S.S.AK,  K.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   127;  263,  1939. 

80.  Liu,  a.   C.   and   A.   Rosenblueth.   Am.  J.   Physiol.    113: 

555.  1935- 

81.  Lockett,  M.  F.  Bril.  J.  Pharmacol.  9:  498,  1954. 

82.  Lockett,  M.  F.  J.  Physiol.  1 33 :  73P,  1956. 

83.  LoEWi,  O.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  189:  239,  1921. 

84.  LoEwi,  O.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  237:  504,  1936. 

85.  LoEWi,  O.  Experientia  12:  331,  1956. 

86.  LoEwi,    O.    AND   H.    Hellauer.    Arch.  ges.    Physiol.    240: 

449,  1938. 

87.  LoEVVi,  O.  AND  E.  N.wratil.  .irch.  ges.  Physiol.  214:  689, 
1 926. 

88.  Luco,  J.  V.  AND  F.  GoNi.  J.  .Kemophysiol.  1 1  :  497,  1948. 

89.  Luco,  J.  V.  AND  H.  Salvestrini.  J.  .N'europhysiol.  5: 
27,  1942. 

go.  LuDUENA,  F.  P.,  B.  F.  Tullar,  L.  v.  Euler  and  A.  M. 


Lands.   A'.V  Internat.   Physiol.   Congr.,  Abslr.   of  Communic: 

30,  "956. 

91.  Lund,  A.  .Acta  Pharmacol,  et  toxicol.  6:  137,  1950. 

92.  Macintosh,  F.  C.  J.  Physiol.  99:  436,  1940. 

93.  Mann,   M.   and  G.   B.  West.   Brit.  J.  Phatmncol.  5:   i^-^, 

■950- 

94.  McDowALL,  R.  J.  S.  J.  Physiol.  104:  41  P,  1945. 

95.  MiNZ,  B.  Arch,  exper.  Path.  u.  Pharmakol.   168;  292,  1932. 

96.  MiNZ,  B.  La  Transmission  Chimique  de  ITiiJtux  .Neri'eux. 
Paris:  Flammarion,  ig47. 

97.  MiN7,  B.  The  Role  of  Humoral  Agents  in  .Nervous  Activity. 
Springfield:  Thomas,  1955. 

98.  Mirkin,  B.   L.   .and  D.  D.  Bonnvcastle.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

178:  5'^9,  1954- 
gg.   MoHME-LuNDiioLM,   E.   Acta  physiol.  scandinav.   29:  suppl. 
108,  1953. 

100.  MoNNIER,  A.  M.  AND  Z.  M.  B.'\CQ.  .Irch.  internal,  physiol. 

40:  485.  1935- 

1 01.  Nachmansohn,  D.  In:  Neurochemisiry,  edited  by  K.  A.  C. 
Elliott,  I.  H.  Page  and  J.  H.  Quastel.  Springfield:  Thomas, 

1955.  P-  39°- 

102.  Nachmansohn,  D.  and  A.  L.  Machado.  J.  Neurophysiol. 

6:  397,  1943- 

103.  Newton,  H.  F.,  R.  L.  Zvvemer  and  \V.  B.  Cannon. 
Am.  J.  Physiol.  96:  377,  1931. 

104.  Orias,  O.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  102:  87,  1932. 

105.  Ostlund,  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  31  :  suppl.  1 12,  1954. 

1 06.  Outschoorn,  a.  S.  and  M.  Voot.  Bril.  J.  Pharmacol.  7 : 

3 '9.  '9.')^- 

107.  Partington,  P.  P.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  117:  55,  1936. 

108.  Peart,  W.  S.  J.  Physiol.   108:  491,   1949. 

log.   Pitkanen,  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  38:  suppl.  129,  1956. 

110.  Raab,  W.  Biochem.  J.  37:  470,  1943. 

111.  Rexed,  B.  and  U.  S.  von  Euler.  .Acta  psychial.  el  iieur. 
scandinav.  26:61,  1951. 

112.  Rosenblueth,  A.  .im.  J.  Physiol.   102:  12,   1932. 

113.  Rosenblueth,  A.  The  Transmission  of  Nerve  Impulses  at 
.Neiiroefjcctor  Junctions  and  Peripheral  Synapses.  New  York : 
Technol.  Press,  Mass.  Inst.  Technol.  and  Wiley,  1950. 

1 14.  Rosenblueth,  A.  and  W.  B.  Cannon.  .Im.  J.  Physiol. 
108:  384,  1934. 

115.  Rosenblueth,  A  and  D  McK.  Rioch.  .Im.  J.  Physiol. 
106:  365,  ig33. 

116.  Schmiterlow,  C.  G.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  16:  suppl.  56, 
1948. 

117.  Schumann,  H.  J.  .-Irch.  cxpei.  Path.  u.  Pharmal.ol.  227: 
566,  1956. 

118.  Snell,   R.   S.    and  J.   R.   Garrett,   .\ature,   London    178: 

1 177,  '956. 

1 19.  Stephenson,  R.  P.  /};;/.  J.  Pharm.  1 1  :  379,  1956. 

120.  Stern,  J.  R.  and  S.  Ochoa.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  179:  491,  1949. 

121.  Stohr,  P.  .Mikroskopische  Anatomic  des  Vegelativen  .\erven- 
syslems.  Berlin:  Springer,   1928. 

122.  Tiffene.\u,  M.  Ann.  Physiol.  10:  535,  1934. 

123.  Trinci,  G.  .Mem.  R.  Accad.  Sc.  Bologna  4:  295,  1907. 

124.  von  Euler,  U.  S.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  12:  73,  1946. 

125.  von  Euler,  U.  S.J.  Physiol.  107:  loP,  1947. 

126.  von  Euler,  U.  S.  Pharmacol.  Rev.  3-  247,  1951. 

127.  von  Euler,  LI.  S.  Pharmacol.  Rev.  6:15,  1954. 


AUTONOMIC    NEUROEFFECTOR    TRANSMISSION 


237 


128.  VON  EuLER,  U.  S.  In:  Neurochemistry,  edited  by  K.  A.  C. 
Elliott,  I.  H.  Page  and  H.J.  Quastel.  Springfield:  Thomas, 

>955- 

129.  VON    EuLER,    U.    S.    Noradrrnalitif;    Chrmisir),    Phynology, 
Pharmacology  and  Clinical  Aspects.  Springfield :  Ttiomas,  1956. 

130.  VON  EuLER,  U.  S.  AND  I.  Floding.  Scandmav.  J.  Clin.  <S 
Lab.  Invest.  In  press. 

131.  VON    EuLER,    U.    S.    AND   N.-A.    HiLLARP.    \alure,   London 

177:  44.  '9,'j''- 


132.  VON  EuLER,  U.  S.  AND  F.  LisHAjKO.  Acta  physiol .  el  Phar- 
macol, neerl.  6:  295,  1957. 

133.  VON   MuRALT,   .\.   Die  Signaliibermitllung  in  .Xerven.   Basel: 
Birkfiauser,  1946. 

134.  VON  Saalfeld,  E.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  235:  15,  1934. 

135.  Weiss,  P.  and  H.  B.  Hiscoe.  J.  exper.  ^ool.  107:  315,  1948. 

136.  Whitelaw,  G.  p.  and  J.  C.  Snyder.  Am.  J.  Physiol,  iio: 

247.  1934- 

137.  ZuPANCIC,  A.  O.  Acta  physiol.  scatidinav.  29:  63,   1953- 


CHAPTER    VIII 


Neuromuscular  transmission  in  invertebrates 


E.  J.    FURS  H  PAN     |     Biophysics  Department,  University  College,  London,  England 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Arthropods 
Crustaceans 

Size  of  the  efferent  nerve  supply 
Polyneuronal  innervation 
Multiterminal  innervation 
Electromechanical  coupling 
The  transmission  process 
The  muscle  spike 
Peripheral  inhibition 
Insects 
Molluscs 
Coelenterates 
Actinozoans 
Scyphozoans 
The  Mechanism  of  Transmission  in  Coelenterates 


TO     PRODUCE     THE     MOST     EFFICIENT     CONTRACTION,     a 

muscle  fiber  must  be  activated  along  its  entire  length 
almost  simultaneously;  otherwise,  the  contracting 
portions  of  the  fiber  must  lengthen  inactive  regions 
before  communicating  their  tension  to  the  tendon. 
That  is,  the  active  parts  of  the  fiber  will  operate  in  a 
less  effective  range  of  the  length-tension  curve.  At 
least  two  mechanisms  are  known  which  can  achieve 
this  relatively  synchronous  excitation  of  the  fiber: 
a)  a  comparatively  rapidly  conducting  muscle  action 
potential,  and  b)  numerous  motor  ner\e  endings  along 
the  length  of  the  muscle  fiber.  The  term  'multi- 
terminal  innervation'  (6i)  will  be  used  to  describe  this 
second  situation.  The  first  device  (conducted  muscle 
action  potential)  is  most  commonly  found  in  the 
skeletal  muscle  of  vertebrates.  The  most  notable 
exception  is  the  case  of  the  slow  muscle  fibers  of  the 
frog  [see  Kuffler  &  Vaughan  Williams  (45)]  in 
which  there  is  multiterminal  innervation  and  a  lack 
of  a  conducted  muscle  spike.    It  should   be  pointed 


out  that  vertebrate  twitch  muscle  fibers  which  exhibit 
such  a  spike  may  have  more  than  one  motor  end- 
plate  (38).  These  cases  should  probably  not  be  in- 
cluded within  the  definition  of  multiterminal  inner- 
vation, however,  for  in  the  absence  of  the  propagated 
muscle  spike,  the  density  of  the  nerve  endings  is  not 
sufficient  to  allow  an  appreciable  contraction. 

The  second  mechanism  (many  nerve  endings)  seems 
to  predominate  in  the  somatic  musculature  of  the 
invertebrates.  An  examination  of  the  evidence  for 
this  type  of  innervation  will  serve  as  one  of  the 
themes  of  this  chapter.  It  will  also  be  interesting  to 
consider  what  differences  in  function  between  the 
two  systems  seem  to  follow  from  the  dissimilarity  in 
the  means  of  spreading  the  muscular  excitation.  A  re- 
lated question  will  also  be  examined — the  other  ana- 
tomical specializations  associated  with  multiterminal 
innervation.  One  such  feature  commonly  found  is 
polyneuronal  innervation  or  the  receipt  by  one  muscle 
fiber  of  more  than  one  motor  axon,  and  of  particular 
interest,  of  motor  axons  which  elicit  from  the  same 
fiber  responses  of  different  strength  and  time  course. 
In  most  vertebrate  muscles,  by  contrast,  the  conducted 
action  potential  is  all-or-nothing  and  produces  a 
stereotyped  twitch.  Thus  different  types  of  contrac- 
tion evoked  by  different  nerve  fibers  do  not  occur. 
Peripheral  inhibition,  as  found  in  certain  inverte- 
brate muscles,  offers  another  example  of  polyneuronal 
innervation  and  this  topic  will  be  considered  as  well. 
Another  question  that  will  be  discussed  concerns  the 
number  of  motor  neurons  which  innervate  whole 
mu.scles.  Here,  too,  a  contrast  with  vertebrate  muscle 
will  be  seen  in  many  cases  and  again  the  dissimilarity 
between  the  ways  in  which  the  excitation  is  spread 
throughout  the  muscle  fiber  can  be  thought  to  under- 
lie these  differences.  That  is,  in  spite  of  the  all-or- 
nothing  contractions,  fine  gradations  of  tension  are 
possible  in  most  vertebrate  twitch  muscles  because  of 


239 


240 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  large  number  of  motor  units.  Where  conducted 
muscle  potentials  are  absent,  however,  gradation  of 
contraction  can  be  effected  by  variations  in  the 
degree  of  excitation  within  each  muscle  fiber.  In  some 
of  these  cases,  therefore,  one  need  not  be  surprised  if 
only  a  few  motor  axons  are  found  to  innervate  whole 
muscles.  The  general  scheme  of  presentation  will  be 
to  consider  together  studies  on  the  same  group  of 
animals. 


ARTHROPODS 

Crustaceans 

Some  of  the  elementary  features  of  crustacean 
nerve-mu.scle  systems  were  known  in  the  nineteenth 
century,  chiefly  from  the  work  of  Biedermann.  In  a 
histological  study  (7)  he  observed  that  the  nerve 
fibers  ramifying  on  the  surface  of  the  opener-muscle 
(abductor  of  the  dactylopodite)  of  the  crayfish  claw 
were  apparently  all  branches  of  only  two  axons. 
These  two  fibers  ran  \ery  close  to  one  another  and 
both  divided  at  each  branch  point  in  approximately 
the  same  place.  The  a.xons  remained  in  juxtaposition 
even  into  the  very  fine  ramifications  on  the  surface 
of  single  muscle  fibers.  Thus  both  axons  seemed  to  in- 
nervate the  same  muscle  fibers.  Mangold  (51)  corrob- 
orated these  findings  and  introduced  the  term  'diplo- 
tomic'  branching  for  this  type  of  concomitant  division 
of  two  nerve  fibers.  He  also  corroborated  the  double 
innervation  of  single  muscle  fibers.  Mangold  saw  that 
as  a  fine  nerve  twig,  consisting  of  two  efferent  axons 
within  a  single  sheath,  approached  the  surface  of  the 
mu.scle  fiber  the  nerve  sheath  joined  and  became 
continuous  with  the  sarcolemma.  Then  the  axons 
would  continue  to  ramify  beneath  the  sarcolemma  and 
thus  come  into  close  contact  with  the  striated  ele- 
ments. He  also  saw  that  more  than  two  axons  might 
divide  together  in  a  manner  analogous  with  diplo- 
tomic  splitting,  and  he  figures  three  axons  doing  so 
on  the  surface  of  the  closer-muscle  (adductor  of  the 
dactylopodite)  of  the  crayfish  claw.  Hoffman  (29) 
examined  the  gross  anatomy  of  the  innervation  of  the 
four  most  distal  muscles  of  the  claw.  One  of  his  un- 
usual observations  was  that  a  single  nerve  fiber  in- 
nervated two  muscles:  an  axon  going  to  the  extensor 
of  the  carpopodite  (stretcher-muscle)  continued  into 
the  next  segment  to  end  in  the  opener-inuscle.  He 
showed  diplotomic  branching  for  the  opener-, 
stretcher-,  and  bender-  (flexor  of  the  carpopodite) 
muscles,  and  found  three  or  more  axons  going  to  the 
closer-muscle.  Hoffman  also  studied  the  distribution 


of  these  various  axons  within  the  two  nerves  which 
run  through  the  length  of  the  claw . 

Knowledge  of  the  differences  in  function  of  the 
two  or  several  axons  innervating  a  single  muscle  also 
goes  back  to  Biedermann.  Richet  (60)  had  observed 
that  with  weak  stimulation  of  the  crayfish  limb  nerve 
the  claw  opened,  and  with  stronger  shocks  it  closed. 
Biedermann  (8)  extended  these  obser\ations.  In  ex- 
periments in  which  he  eliminated  one  or  the  other  of 
the  two  muscles  (opener-  or  closer-muscle)  operating 
the  dactyl,  he  was  still  able  to  obtain  opening  or  clos- 
ing depending  on  the  strength  of  nerse  stimulation; 
and  if  electrodes  placed  at  the  muscle  evoked  con- 
traction, excitation  of  the  nerve  could  inhibit  this 
contraction.  Biedermann  correctly  concluded  that 
there  must  have  been  separate  nerve  fibers  to  mediate 
the  inhibitory  process;  and  since  the  opener-muscle, 
for  example,  receixed  only  two  axons,  one  of  these 
was  designated  the  excitor  and  the  other  the  in- 
hibitor. 

Lucas  provided  information  on  the  function  of  the 
additional  axon  in  a  muscle  with  a  triple  innersation. 
He  observed  (49)  both  slow  and  twitch  contractions 
in  the  closer-muscle  of  the  loijster  claw;  and  further 
found  that  the  strength-duration  curve  for  indirect 
stimulation  (by  way  of  the  nerve)  showed  a  sharp 
discontinuity  at  the  point  at  which  one  type  of  con- 
traction was  replaced  by  the  other.  From  these,  and 
additional  experiments  on  the  crayfish  claw  (50), 
Lucas  concluded  that  the  two  contraction  tvpes  were 
e\oked  by  stimulation  of  different  'substances'  (types 
of  nerve  element.s). 

This  conclusion  has  been  amph  confirmed  and  it  is 
now  known  that  more  than  one  t\  pe  of  motor  axon 
can  inner\ate  a  single  crustacean  muscle  and  that 
the  electrical  and  mechanical  responses  e\'oked  by  each 
can  differ  with  respect  to  amplitude,  time  course, 
facilitation  and  fatigue.  The  most  convincing  demon- 
stration of  this  was  pro\ided  by  van  Harre\eld  & 
VViersma  (74)  who  succeeded  in  isolating  and  stimulat- 
ing single  functioning  motor  axons  from  crayfish  limb 
nerves.  In  particular  they  studied  ner\e  fibers  exoking 
contraction  of  the  closer-muscle.  In  normal  prepara- 
tions (but  not  in  regenerated  claws)  two  such  axons 
were  always  found  and  stimulation  of  the  remainder 
of  the  nerve  did  not  result  in  contraction  of  this 
muscle.  A  single  impulse  in  the  thicker  of  the  two 
axons  evoked  a  rapid  twitch,  while  a  train  of  impulses 
was  needed  in  the  thinner  fiber  to  produce  even  a 
small  contraction.  About  30  sec.  were  required  to  at- 
tain maximal  tension  during  stimulation  of  the  thin 
axon  at  40  shocks  per  sec.  This  time  for  the  thick 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


241 


nerve  fiber  was  only  about  i  sec.  and  the  terms 
'slow'  and  'fast'  were  therefore  used  to  describe  the 
two  contractions.  These  same  terms  were  then  ex- 
tended to  the  concomitant  muscle  potentials  and  to 
the  axons  themselves  (e.g.  'slow  a.xon').  The  muscle 
potentials  showed  even  greater  dififerences,  the  slow 
potentials  being  very  small  but  augmenting  with 
repetitive  stimulation.  Despite  this  increase  (facilita- 
tion) they  remained  smaller  than  a  single  fast  muscle- 
potential.  In  the  crayfish  closer  muscle,  which  is  per- 
haps not  typical  in  this  respect,  the  fast  potential 
normally  exhibited  no  facilitation. 

SIZE     OF    THE     EFFERENT    NERVE     SUPPLY.     One     of    the 

questions  considered  subsequently  by  these  same  au- 
thors and  their  collaborators  concerned  the  numbers 
of  excitatory  and  inhibitory  axons  supplying  the 
seven  most  distal  limb  mu.scles  in  different  species  of 
decapod  crustaceans.  The  latter  included  crayfish, 
lobsters,  rock  lobsters,  crabs,  hermit  crabs  and  various 
other  anomurans.  The  techniques  involved  isolation 
and  stimulation,  separately,  of  as  many  of  the  axons 
having  an  excitatory  or  inhibitory  effect  upon  a  par- 
ticular inuscle  as  could  be  found,  and  an  additional 
estimate  of  the  minimum  number  of  efferent  axons 
using  methylene-blue  staining.  All  liinb  muscles  that 
were  examined  received  at  least  two  axons  and  at  most 
five,  and  of  the.se  at  least  one  was  always  an  inhibitor. 
The  different  groups  of  decapods  showed  diversity  in 
the  numbers  of  inhibitory  nerve  fibers  supplying  a 
particular  muscle,  but  the  numbers  of  excitatory 
axons  were  constant  from  species  to  species.  The  fol- 
lowing table  summarizes  the  findings  for  motor  deca- 
pod axons  only  (74,  75,  76). 

Note  that  Hoffman's  histological  observation  that 
one  axon  supplied  two  muscles  was  confirmed  by 
physiological  methods.  In  addition  to  the  nerve  fibers 
listed  in  table  i,  each  muscle  receives  at  least  one 
inhibitory  axon  and  in  some  species  there  are  muscles 
receiving  two  (78).  Further,  it  is  typical  for  an  in- 
hibitory axon  to  supply  more  than  one  muscle  (up  to 
seven),  and  variations  in  the  particular  muscles  sup- 
plied by  a  common  inhibitor  were  also  found.  Wiersma 
&  Ripley  (85)  have  summarized  the  inhibitory  in- 
nervation of  these  muscles. 

POLYNEURON.'>iL  INNERVATION.  From  the  early  histo- 
logical work  and  the  fact  that  inhibition  can  counter- 
act excitation,  it  was  apparent  that  motor  and  in- 
hibitory axons  both  innervate  the  same  muscle  fibers. 
Can  a  muscle  fiber  also  receive  more  than  one  excita- 
tory axon?  There  is  now  convincing  evidence,  both 


TABLE   I.  Anmlwr  oj  Excitatory  Axons  Supplying  the  Seven 
Most  Distal  Limh  Muscles  of  Decapod  Crustaceans 


Muscle 
(common  name) 

Segment  Moved 

Type  of 
Movement 

No.  of 
Motor 
Axons 

Opener 

Dactylopodite 

Abduction 
(or  exten- 
sion) 

I* 

Closer 

Dactylopodite 

Adduction 
(or  flexion) 

2 

Stretcher 

Propodite 

Extension 

I* 

Bender 

Propodite 

Flexion 

2 

Main  flexor 

Carpopodite 

Flexion 

4 

Accesory 

Carpopodite 

Flexion 

1 

flexor 

Extensor 

Carpopodite 

Extension 

2 

*  This  same 

axon  innervates  both 

muscles. 

histological  (73)  and  physiological,  that  this  is  indeed 
possible.  Although  the  observation  is  attended  by 
some  difficulties,  the  fibers  of  the  closer-muscle  have 
been  seen  to  contract  following  stimulation  of  either 
the  fast  or  slow  axon  (72).  Wiersma  &  van  Harreveld 
(84)  showed  that  stimulation  of  one  of  the  motor 
axons  to  the  closer-muscle  augmented  a  contraction 
evoked  shortly  afterwards  by  stimulation  of  the  other 
(heterofacilitation).  No  mutual  influence  with  respect 
to  fatigue  or  facilitation  of  the  muscle  action  poten- 
tials, however,  could  be  demonstrated.  These  results 
suggested  the  presence  of  a  pathway  susceptible  to 
mutual  influence  .somewhere  between  muscle  action 
potential  and  contraction;  and  the  conclusion  was 
drawn  that  at  least  .some  of  the  contractile  substance 
was  activated  by  both  axons.  More  recently  Fatt  & 
Katz  (22)  used  intracellular  electrodes  to  demon- 
strate that  muscle  potentials  were  evoked  in  the 
same  fibers  by  both  fast  and  slow  axons,  and  it  was 
shown  that  the  potentials  could  simimate.  There  is  no 
good  quantitative  determination  of  what  percentage 
of  the  fibers  in  a  muscle  receiving  two  excitatory 
axons  are  innervated  by  both  axons. 

van  Harreveld  &  Wiersma  (76)  have  al.so  consid- 
ered the  question  of  polyneuronal  innervation  in  a 
muscle  with  four  motor  axons  (and  one  inhibitor). 
Here,  too,  they  found  that  a  test  contraction  set  up  by 
stimulation  of  one  of  the  four  axons  was  slightly  aug- 
mented by  previous  stimulation  of  any  of  the  other 
three  motor  axons  (heterofacilitation).  Their  experi- 
ments did  not  provide  direct  evidence,  however,  con- 
cerning the  numbers  of  axons  supplying  each  muscle 
fiber.  More  recently,  Furshpan  (26)  has  studied  the 
same  muscle  (main  flexor  of  the  carpopodite  in  the 


242 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


California  rock  lobster),  recording  from  the  muscle 
fibers  with  intracellular  electrodes  while  stimulating 
the  four  motor  axons  separately.  In  this  way  20  or  30 
fibers  in  each  preparation  were  sampled  and  if,  upon 
stimulation  of  a  particular  axon,  a  monophasic 
muscle  potential  in  the  direction  of  depolarization 
was  recorded  at  the  microelectrode,  it  was  concluded 
that  the  impaled  fiber  was  innervated  by  the  axon  in 
question.  While  reasons  were  presented  for  believing 
that  the  sampling  was  not  entirely  random,  the  fol- 
lowing results  were  recognized.  Some  of  the  muscle 
fibers  did  receive  all  four  of  the  motor  axons  (and  pre- 
sumably the  inhibitor  as  well);  these  were,  however,  a 
small  minority  comprising  about  5  to  10  per  cent  of 
those  tested.  The  remaining  fibers  were  approximately 
equally  distributed  among  tho.se  innervated  by  one, 
two,  or  three  of  the  motor  axons,  and  different  com- 
binations of  two  and  three  axons  were  found.  Further, 
the  size  of  the  muscle  potential  which  stimulation  of 
a  nerve  fiber  evoked  could  vary  considerably  from 
muscle  fiber  to  muscle  fiber.  Thus  this  muscle  shows 
remarkable  heterogeneity  with  respect  to  the  number, 
combination  and  effectiveness  of  the  motor  axons 
which  supply  its  individual  fibers.  This  is  a  factor 
which  it  is  important  to  consider  in  attempting  to 
correlate  muscle  potentials  and  contraction  and  em- 
phasizes the  desirability  of  performing  such  experi- 
ments on  single  muscle  fibers. 

It  is  not  known  to  what  extent  this  analysis  applies 
to  the  simpler  case  of  a  muscle  receiving  only  two 
motor  axons,  but  from  experiments  cited  above,  and 
from  some  incomplete  work  with  intracellular  elec- 
trodes (unpublished  observations),  it  seems  as  if  most 
fibers  receive  both  axons.  Here,  too,  there  is  hetero- 
geneity with  respect  to  the  size  of  the  muscle  poten- 
tials evoked  and  some  fibers  have  been  found  in  which 
the  'slow'  axon  elicited  much  larger  responses  than 
did  the  'fast' ;  more  work  is  needed,  however. 

MULTiTERMiN.^L  INNERVATION.  In  a  histological  Study 
d'Ancona  (14)  encountered  considerable  variation 
among  different  crustaceans,  but  in  extreme  cases  he 
observed  numerous  nerve  terminals  on  single  muscle 
fibers,  at  times  to  the  extent  of  an  ending  for  each 
sarcomere  (cf.  41).  van  Harreveld  (73)  has  also  de- 
scribed numerous  axon  terminations  on  single  fibers. 
In  one  particularly  good  preparation  (from  the 
opener-muscle  of  crayfish)  he  observed  28  nerve  end- 
ings on  one  muscle  fiber,  the  latter  being  3.5  mm 
long;  it  is  not  known  whether  this  density  of  endings 
is  tvpical.  Because  of  the  numerous  terminations  on 


each  muscle  fiber  and  the  fact  that  even  small  nerve 
branches  may  run  for  considerable  distances  on  the 
fiber  surfaces,  a  dense  tree  of  intramuscular  nerve 
elements  was  observed,  van  Harreveld  (72)  described 
this  as  a  'feltwork'.  Holmes  (30),  however,  contested 
these  observations  and  showed  that  connective  tissue 
around  the  mu.scle  fibers  could  be  made  to  stain  to 
give  the  appearance  of  a  'feltwork'.  While  this  is  un- 
doubtedly so,  it  is  very  unlikely  that  the  structures 
which  van  Harreveld  observed  were  not  terminals 
since  he  was  able  to  follow  them  back  to  larger  axon 
branches,  and  since  in  the  opener-muscle  with  its 
double  innervation,  he  saw  two  fibers  terminating  to- 
gether at  each  ending. 

Another  controversial  point  which  became  asso- 
ciated with  the  question  of  multiterminal  innervation 
concerned  the  presence  of  conducted  muscle  action 
potentials  of  the  type  found  in  the  vertebrates.  Because 
the  potentials  which  were  usually  recorded  with  ex- 
ternal electrodes  summated  and  were  monophasic, 
Wiersma  &  van  Harreveld  generally  propounded  the 
view  that  such  propagated  potentials  would  not  be 
found  in  crustacean  muscle  and  that  the  spread  of 
excitation  would  be  effected  solely  by  the  numerous 
nerve  endings  (79,  82).  In  1946,  Katz  &  Kuffler  (42) 
were  able,  however,  to  record  in  crayfish  and  crabs 
diphasic  muscle  action  potentials  which  were  propa- 
gated at  a  velocity  of  about  20  cm  per  sec.  The  con- 
ducted spikes  were  seen  to  arise  from  summating  po- 
tentials resembling  the  vertebrate  endplate  potential. 
The  summating  potentials  were  recorded  from  only 
circum.scribed  regions  of  the  muscle  fibers,  thus  pro- 
viding physiological  evidence  against  multiterminal 
innervation.  Twitches  were  seen  to  accompany  spikes, 
while  with  the  local-type  potentials  slower  smoother 
contractions  were  observed.  Thus  they  viewed  crus- 
tacean mu.scle  as  essentially  similar  to  that  of  the 
vertebrates,  but  with  some  quantitative  differences: 
namelv,  that  in  the  crustaceans  the  safety  factor  for 
transmission  would  be  lower  so  that  facilitation  and 
recruitment  would  be  more  important  and  the  eflicacy 
of  local  endplate  potentials  in  evoking  contraction 
would  be  enhanced. 

These  two  views  were  subsequently  reconciled  by 
Fatt  &  Katz  who  clearly  showed,  using  intracellular 
recording  electrodes,  the  presence  of  conducted 
muscle  spikes  (20)  but,  on  the  other  hand,  also  gave 
a  physiological  demonstration  of  multiterminal  in- 
nervation (22).  In  the  latter  paper  they  reported  that, 
wherever  a  microelectrode  entered  a  muscle  fiber,  an 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


243 


"endplate  potential'  (e.p.p.')  could  be  recorded  fol- 
lowing nerve  stimulation.  Further,  they  were  able  to 
enter  a  given  muscle  fiber  at  a  number  of  points  along 
several  millimeters  of  its  length  and  to  measure  the 
amount  of  variation  in  the  amplitudes  of  the  e.p.p.'s. 
This  variation  was  comparatively  small  and  the  dura- 
tion of  the  rising  phase  of  the  e.p.p.'s  was  practically 
constant  along  the  length  of  the  fiber.  These  findings 
should  be  compared  with  the  situation  in,  for  ex- 
ample, frog  twitch  muscle  fibers  (19).  The  earlier 
results  (42)  which  indicated  the  presence  of  only  lo- 
calized e.p.p.'s  were  probably  due  to  partial  denerva- 
tion of  the  fibers  in  dissecting  the  'strip'  preparations 
that  were  used. 

Despite  this  clarification,  major  problems  of  crus- 
tacean neuromuscular  transmission  remain.  Little  is 
known  about  the  relative  effectiveness  of  the  different 
e.p.p.'s  which  can  be  set  up  in  one  muscle  fiber,  or  of 
the  spike,  in  evoking  contraction.  The  mechanism 
whereby  the  nerve  impulse  leads  to  an  e.p.p.  is  very 
incompletely  understood.  The  spike,  too,  has  some 
curious  attributes  which  warrant  further  study.  Each 
of  these  problems  will  be  considered  in  turn. 

ELECTROMECHANICAL  COUPLING.  This  topic  is  perhaps 
outside  of  the  subject  of  neuromuscular  transmission 
in  the  usual  sense  of  that  phrase.  In  inany  of  the 
experiments  which  have  been  performed,  however, 
both  the  electrical  and  mechanical  responses  to  nerve 
stimulation  were  recorded  and  some  of  the  observa- 
tions concerning  the  latter  will  be  considered  briefly. 
It  has  been  found  by  Wiersma  &  van  Harreveld  (83) 
that  in  certain  muscles  (e.g.  the  claw-closer  of 
Blepharipoda  occidentalism  low-frequency  stimulation  (10 
to  1 5  shocks  per  sec.)  applied  to  the  fast  axon  could 
elicit  large  muscle  potentials  unaccompanied  by  any 
visible  contraction;  while  a  stimulus  of  the  same  fre- 
quency delivered  to  the  slow  axon  evoked  much 
smaller  muscle  potentials  which,  nevertheless,  set 
up  a  contraction.  Reasons  were  presented  for  believ- 
ing that  this  seemingly  paradoxical  behavior  could 
occur  within  a  single  muscle  fiber.  Inasmuch,  how- 
ever, as  the  electrical  recording  was  made  with  ex- 
ternal electrodes  and  the  contraction  ol3ser\ed  was 
that  of  the  whole  muscle,  it  is  not  possiljle  to  conclude 
definitely   that   this  was  the  case.   The  unequivocal 

'  There  is  some  disadvantage  in  using  the  term  e.p.p.'  since 
it  suggests  the  presence  of  a  particular  anatomical  structure, 
which  is  probably  absent  in  the  crustaceans,  and  suggests  a 
functional  similarity  with  the  \ertebrate  neuromuscular  junc- 
tion, the  actual  extent  of  which  is  unknown. 


demonstration  of  this  phenomenon  will  probably 
have  to  be  made  on  single  muscle  fibers.  The  possible 
presence  of  such  a  phenomenon  is  interesting  because 
it  would  seem  to  suggest  that  the  transmitter  has 
some  other  effect  aside  from  that  which  is  manifested 
in  the  change  in  muscle  membrane  potential.  A  re- 
lated observation  has  been  made  by  Kuffler  (43)  on 
the  muscles  of  the  fast  and  slow  stretch  receptors  of 
the  crayfish  abdomen.  This  preparation  has  the  ad- 
vantage that  the  muscle  bundle  is  very  small  and  can 
be  isolated,  with  its  nerve  supply,  and  observed  under 
a  microscope  with  transmitted  light,  and  that  at  the 
same  time  a  fiber  can  be  impaled  with  a  micro- 
electrode  for  intracellular  recording  of  inembrane 
potential.  Ii  the  fast  muscle  bundle  he  observed  that 
if  a  nerve  impulse  evoked  only  an  e.p.p.  (which  was 
usually  10  to  25  mv  in  amplitude)  no  contraction  was 
visible.  Only  if  the  e.p.p.  gave  rise  to  a  spike  was  there 
visible  contraction,  and  then  a  rapid  twitch  was  the 
result.  In  the  slow  muscle  bundle,  however,  only 
e.p.p.'s  (5  to  15  mv)  were  observed  and  these  were 
accompanied  by  contraction.  The  failure  of  the  fast 
e.p.p.'s  to  bring  about  any  visible  muscle  shortening 
is  puzzling  in  view  of  the  fact  that  they  are  distributed 
along  the  length  of  this  muscle  by  numerous  nerve 
endings  and  attain  a  size  which  may  be  a  considerable 
fraction  of  the  spike  amplitude.  Wiersma  (80)  has 
also  recently  published  observations  made  on  lobster 
closer-muscles  which  indicate  that  fast  e.p.p.'s  may 
fail  to  bring  about  contraction,  while  spikes  succeed 
in  doing  so. 

THE  TRANSMISSION  PROCESS.  It  scems  vcry  likely  that 
the  transfer  of  excitation  across  the  crustacean  neuro- 
muscular junction  is  effected  by  a  chemical  trans- 
mitter, rather  than  by  the  passive  flow  of  the  nerve 
action  current.  While  the  total  number  of  nerve 
endings  may  be  large  (73),  in  any  given  section  of 
muscle  fiber  the  ratio  of  areas  of  axon  membrane  to 
muscle  fiber  meml^rane  is  probably  always  quite  small. 
Thus  there  is  little  current  from  the  nerve  terminals 
available  for  charging  the  capacitance  of  the  muscle 
fiber,  which  is  particularly  large  (approximately  40  /if 
per  cm-)  in  Crustacea  (20).  There  are  some  analogies 
with  the  vertebrate  neuromuscular  mechanism  which 
are  only  suggestive;  during  repetitive  stimulation  of 
the  nerve,  random,  often  large,  variations  in  the  size 
of  the  e.p.p.  can  be  seen  (22).  These  fluctuations 
might  be  caused  by  intermittent  failure  of  conduction 
in  some  of  the  terminal  nerve  branches;  but  it  is  also 
possible  that  they  represent  a  quantal  release  mecha- 


244 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


nism  for  the  transmitter.  Spontaneous  miniature 
e.p.p.'s  have  not,  however,  been  oljserved  (see  Chap- 
ter VII).  Facihtation,  which  at  the  vertebrate  endplate 
is  also  a  property  of  the  transmitter-release  mecha- 
nism (i6),  is  usually  present  and  often  striking  at  crus- 
tacean nerve-muscle  junctions  (42,  83). 

The  chemical  identity  of  the  crustacean  neuro- 
muscular transmitter(s)  is  unknown.  That  it  is  not 
acetylcholine  or  a  closely  related  compound  seems 
\  ery  likely  from  a  number  of  experiments  (3,  1 7,  40). 
Wright  (88)  was  able  to  obtain  blocking  in  the  cray- 
fish closer-muscle  with  curare  and  dihydroerythroidin, 
but  perfusion  with  solutions  containing  approximately 
io~^  and  5  X  lo""*  gm  per  ml,  respectively,  was  neces- 
sary. Other  physiologically  active  compounds  that 
have  been  found  to  have  little  or  no  effect  on  crus- 
tacean neuromuscular  junctions  are  trimethylamine, 
trimethylaminoxide,  tyraminc  (40),  choline  (see, 
however,  20  and  below),  mecholsl,  carbachol,  musca- 
rine, strychnine,  pilocarpine,  dioitalin,  epinephrine, 
nicotine,  caffeine  and  rotenone  (17).  Although  a  num- 
ber of  drugs,  such  as  local  anesthetics  and  veratrine, 
were  found  to  affect  the  response  of  the  nerve-muscle 
system,  their  action  was  probably  on  the  nerves  for 
the  most  part. 

THE  MUSCLE  SPIKE.  As  mentioned  above,  an  e.p.p.  of 
sufficient  size  will,  at  least  in  some  fibers,  evoke  an 
additional  membrane  response,  the  spike  potential. 
This  spike  differs  in  several  aspects  from,  for  example, 
the  conducted  action  potential  of  frog  twitch  muscle 
fibers.  Although  it  may  overshoot  the  resting  poten- 
tial, the  inside  of  the  muscle  fiber  becoming  rela- 
tively positive  during  its  peak,  often  this  overshoot  is 
absent.  The  spike  is  commonly  not  all-or-nothing,  its 
height  varying  continuously  with  the  size  of  the  de- 
polarization (20)  or  e.p.p.  (26)  evoking  it.  It  may  be 
nonpropagated  and,  when  conduction  occurs,  the 
velocity  is  low  (25  to  40  cm  per  sec).  A  striking 
property  of  the  spike  was  discovered  (20)  during  an 
attempt  to  replace  the  sodium  ions  of  the  external 
fluid  by  an  'inert'  cation.  Substitution  of  choline  for 
the  sodium  unexpectedh-  resulted  in  an  increase  in 
the  size  and  duration  of  the  spike.  Fibers  which  had 
previously  shown  only  local  spikes  exhibited  large 
conducted  ones  in  the  choline  medium.  Even  more 
striking  increases  were  oijtained  with  other  quar- 
ternary  ammonium  compounds  (e.g.  tetraethyl-  and 
tetrabutylammonium).  With  the  latter  (TBA)  in- 
creases in  spike  duration  of  several  hundred  times  were 
common  and  action  potentials  lasting  up  to  18  sec. 
were  observed.  The  TBA  effect  was  irreversible  and. 


surprisingly,  even  after  a  preparation  which  had 
been  exposed  to  this  drug  was  washed  with  a  solution 
containing  no  .sodium  and  no  TBA  (but  with  sucrose 
and  excess  magnesium)  large  long-lasting  spikes  were 
still  observed.  Experiments  to  determine  if  it  were 
the  ammonium  compounds  themselves  which  were 
carrying  the  current  during  the  spike  were  incon- 
clusive. It  was  noticed  that  during  prolonged  ex- 
posures to  TBA  the  resting  potential  decreased,  ac- 
companied, however,  by  an  increase  in  membrane 
resistance.  The  possibility  was  therefore  considered 
that  TBA  reduces  potassium  conductance.  Although 
such  an  assumption  could  help  to  explain  the  enor- 
mous prolongation  of  the  spike,  the  identity  of  the  ion 
carrying  the  inward  current  during  the  action  po- 
tential is  still  unknown. - 

PERiPHER.-^L  INHIBITION.  It  is  now  known  that  the 
inhibition  described  above  is  mediated  by  separate 
peripheral  axons  which  run  and  branch  with  the 
motor  axons.  A  direct  demonstration  of  this  was  pro- 
\ided  b>-  van  Harreveld  &  Wiersma  (75)  who  were 
able  to  isolate,  as  single  functioning  axons,  the  mcJtor 
and  inhibitory  nerve  fibers  to  several  muscles  in  the 
crayfish  cheliped.  It  was  found  that  contraction 
evoked  by  the  first  type  of  axon  could  be  reduced  or 
abolished  by  concomitant  stimulation  of  the  second. 
The  inhibitor  was  more  effective  in  suppressing  slow 
than  fast  contractions,  and  in  some  muscles  the 
latter  were  unaffected  by  inhibitory  stimulation  (53). 
A  study  of  the  comparative  effectiveness  of  the  in- 
hibitors to  various  muscles  in  a  number  of  species 
was  made  (81).  The  results  were  expressed  in  terms 
of  the  ratio  of  the  frequency  of  inhibitory-axon  to 
that  of  excitatory-axon  stimulation  when  the  former 
was  just  sufficient  to  suppress  all  contraction.  The 
most  effectively  inhibited  motor  system  found  was 
the  slow  contraction  of  the  bender-muscle  of  Pachy- 
grapsis  crassipes  in  which  this  ratio  was  about  one- 
third.  Fast  systems  most  usually  had  values  of  this 
ratio  ai:>ove,  slow  systems  below,  unity. 

A  surprising  result  was  obtained  when  the 
electrical,  as  well  as  the  mechanical,  events  were  re- 
corded during  inhibition.  It  was  found  (44,  53)  that 
contraction  could  be  completely  suppressed,  while  the 
muscle  potentials  might  be  reduced  by  a  variable 
amount  or  apparently  not  at  all.  The  extent  to  which 
the  e.p.p.'s  were  affected  depended  upon  the  relative 

-  Evidence  that  calcium  ions  carry  this  current  has  recently 
been  obtained  by  P.  Fatt  &  B.  Ginsborg  (J.  Physiol.  142:  516, 
>958)- 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


245 


times  of  arrival  of  the  inhibitory  and  motor  nerve 
impulses.  Maximum  reduction  (to  aljout  20  per  cent) 
occurred  when  the  former  slightly  preceded  the 
latter;  and  no  effect  on  the  e.p.p.'s  was  seen  if  the 
inhibitory  impulse  arrived  much  after  the  excitatory. 
Thus,  it  was  suggested  that  inhibition  could  act  in 
two  places:  on  some  process  a)  between  nerve  im- 
pulse and  muscle  potential,  and  6)  between  muscle 
potential  and  contraction. 

More  recently,  changes  in  the  muscle  membrane 
during  inhibition  have  been  studied  by  Fatt  &  Katz 
(21)  using  intracellular  electrodes.  They  confirmed 
previous  results  that  the  e.p.p.  can  be  greatly  reduced 
during  inhibition  and  that  the  extent  of  the  reduc- 
tion depends  upon  the  relative  timing  of  the  inhibitory 
and  motor  nerve  impulses.  In  order  to  test  for  other 
postjunctional  effects  of  inhibitory  impulses,  two 
microelectrodes  were  inserted  into  the  same  muscle 
fiber,  one  in  a  recording  circuit  for  measuring  mem- 
brane potential  and  the  other  connected  to  a  current 
generator  for  the  purpose  of  altering  the  level  of  the 
membrane  potential.  Then  it  was  found  that  inhibi- 
tory nerve  impulses  did  not  result  in  any  detectable 
postjunctional  potential  changes  if  the  membrane 
potential  was  at  a  certain  level,  usually  at  or  near  the 
resting  potential;  but  if  the  membrane  potential  were 
displaced,  by  passing  current  through  the  other  intra- 
cellular electrode,  inhibitory  nerve  impulses  were  fol- 
lowed by  transient  muscle  potentials,  similar  to,  but 
slower  than,  e.p.p.'s.  They  were  referred  to  as  I-po- 
tentials  and  could  appear  either  as  transient  hyper- 
polarizations  or  depolarizations  depending  upon 
whether  the  resting  membrane  potential  had  been 
decreased  or  increased,  respectively,  by  the  current 
passed  through  the  second  microelectrode.  That  is, 
the  I-potentials  were  seen  as  reductions  of  any  dis- 
placement of  the  membrane  potential  from  some 
equilibrium  level,  usually  close  to  the  resting  po- 
tential. These  are  the  effects  which  would  be  expected 
if  the  event  underlying  the  I-potentials  was  a  tran- 
sient increase  in  some  fraction  of  membrane  conduct- 
ance and,  in  particular,  the  conductance  of  some 
species  of  ions  having  an  equilibrium  potential  equal 
to  the  membrane  potential  at  which  no  I-potential 
appears.  K+  or  Cl~  might,  therefore,  be  the  ions  in- 
volved. Although  the  conductance  change  underlying 
the  I-potential  does  tend  to  reduce  any  deviation  of 
the  membrane  potential  (including  an  e.p.p.)  from 
an  equilibrium  potential  near  to  the  resting  potential 
and  would  thus  serve  as  an  inhibitory  mechanism,  the 
effect  was  found  to  be  insufficient  to  account  quanti- 
tatively for   all   of  the   inhibition   actually   observed. 


Another  mechanism  was  therefore  suggested  in  which 
the  inhibitory  and  excitatory  transmitter  substances 
would  specifically  antagonize  one  another  at  the 
receptor  sites  on  the  muscle  membrane.  The  original 
observation  that  contraction  can  be  abolished  without 
any  reduction  in  the  size  of  the  e.p.p.'s  still  awaits  con- 
firmation using  intracellular  recording  of  potential 
while  observing  contraction  of  the  same  fiber  (cf.  18). 

Insects 

There  have  been  fewer  physiological  studies  of  in- 
sect than  of  crustacean  motor  systems,  but  there  seem 
to  be  many  resemblances  between  the  neuromuscular 
mechanisms  of  these  two  groups,  as  well  as  some  inter- 
esting differences.  Among  the  latter,  one  should  note 
the  apparent  lack  of  peripheral  inhibitory  axons  in  the 
insects.  Also,  the  histological  appearance  of  the  motor 
ner\e  endings  can  be,  at  least  superficially,  different 
from  that  in  the  crustaceans;  for  in  many  insect 
species,  rather  than  continuously  tapering  to  sub- 
microscopic  diiTiensions,  the  nerve  terminals  present 
an  enlarged  bulbous  or  conical  appearance.  Accord- 
ing to  Marcu  (52),  however,  these  structures,  re- 
ferred to  as  Doyere's  cones  (cf.  25),  may  only  repre- 
sent a  sudden  and  profuse  branching  of  the  nerve 
ending  in  which  the  individual  twiglets  are  not  always 
seen.  Marcu  also  studied  a  species  of  orthopteran  in 
which  the  manner  of  branching  of  the  nerve  was  more 
similar  to  the  situation  in  crustaceans.  Hoyle  (35), 
working  with  the  locust,  has  observed  what  may  have 
been  the  terminal  apparatus  still  attached  to  the  final 
nerve  branch  after  pulling  the  latter  free  from  the 
muscle  fiber.  The  axons,  probably  beyond  the  place 
at  which  they  had  entered  the  sarcolemma,  were  con- 
tinuous with  a  branched  claw-shaped  structure  which 
spread  over  an  area  20  to  30  y.  in  diameter. 

The  insects  also  show  a  difference  from  the  crus- 
taceans in  the  gross  organization  of  the  muscle.  For 
example,  Hoyle  (35)  observed  that  the  muscle  fibers 
of  the  locust  were  organized  into  muscle  bundles 
each  of  which  received  separate  nerve  and  tracheal 
branches.  He  referred  to  these  bundles  as  'muscle 
units'.  In  some  muscles  this  type  of  arrangement  did 
not  seem  to  signify  any  fundamental  difference  from 
the  crustacean  situation.  For  example,  the  extensor 
tibia  is  innervated  by  three  efferent  axons  and  the 
branches  from  two  or  all  three  of  the  axons  supply 
each  muscle  unit.  But  in  the  flexor  tibia,  each  of  the 
five  or  six  muscle  units  was  supplied  by  separate 
neurons.  The  fibers  of  a  unit  may  receive,  however, 
more  than  one  axon.  Working  with  the  same  muscle 


246 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


in  another  orthopteran  QRomalea),  Ripley  (61)  found 
at  least  six  steps  of  contraction  strength  as  he  increased 
the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  applied  to  the  motor 
nerve.  Of  these,  four  represented  twitches  and  two, 
slow  contractions.  It  seems  likely  that  at  least  the  four 
twitch  contractions  involved  different  sets  of  muscle 
fibers. 

The  points  of  similarity  between  the.se  two  groups 
of  arthropods  include  the  small  number  of  motor 
nerve  fibers  (35,  51,  58,  61,  62),  multiterminal  (15, 
25,  36)  and  polyneuronal  (36,  51,  58)  innervation  of 
single  muscle  fibers,  and  different  contraction  t)pes 
(fast  and  slow)  evoked  from  the  same  muscle  by  stimu- 
lation of  different  axons  (36,  58,  61).  The  abo\e 
references  are  to  both  histological  and  physiological 
studies,  and  the  evidence  derived  from  the  latter  will 
be  considered  in  greater  detail.  For  example,  Pringle 
(58)  was  able  to  distinguish  twitch  and  slow  contrac- 
tions in  the  flexor  tibia  of  the  cockroach  and  observed 
visually  that  the  same  muscle  fibers  could  be  involved 
in  either  type  of  contraction.  There  have  been  differ- 
ent explanations  offered  for  these  two  contraction 
types.  Pringle,  on  the  one  hand,  has  suggested  that 
the  distinction  between  twitch  and  slow  contraction 
would  lie  in  the  number  of  muscle  fibers  activated  ijy 
the  two  types  of  motor  axons.  Thus,  he  attributed  the 
facilitation  observable  in  the  slow  system  to  a  pro- 
gressive recruitment  of  additional  muscle  fibers, 
each  fiber  giving  an  all-or-nothing  response.  Wilson 
(86),  on  the  other  hand,  has  recently  studied  the  same 
muscle,  using  intracellular  electrodes,  and  suggested 
that  the  two  contraction  types  occur  in  two  different 
groups  of  muscle  fibers,  much  as  in  the  slow  and 
twitch  systems  of  the  frog.  His  evidence,  however,  was 
indirect  inasmuch  as  the  contraction  of  the  impaled 
fibers  was  not  recorded,  and  the  motor  axons  were 
not  separately  stimulated.  There  is  yet  a  third  pos- 
sible explanation  for  the  two  contraction  types  in  in- 
sect muscle.  According  to  this  explanation,  the  fast 
axon  would  give  rise  to  a  fast  muscle  action  potential 
(spike)  which  would  evoke  a  twitch;  while  the  slow 
axon,  innervating  many  of  the  same  muscle  fibers, 
would  give  rise  to  a  slower,  smaller,  facilitating 
muscle  potential,  the  mechanical  response  to  which 
would  ije  a  slow  smooth  contraction.  This  is  the 
mechanism  which  seems  largely  to  explain  the  fast 
and  slow  systems  in  the  crustaceans  (see  above).  It  is 
known  that  this  mechanism  must  be  present  to  some 
extent,  and  is  probably  the  most  important  of  the 
three,  although  the  other  two  may  operate  as  well. 
Both  in  crustaceans  and  in  insects,  however,  there  is 
the  possibility  of  slow  potentials  exokint;  spikes  and 


thereby  twitches  so  that  recruitment  may  occur  in  the 
slow  as  well  as  in  the  fast  systems,  and  further  there 
may  be  muscle  fibers  innervated  solely  by  either  fast 
or  slow  a.xons. 

Evidence  for  fast  and  slow  potentials  occurring  in 
the  same  fibers  of  insect  muscle  has  recently  been  pro- 
\ided  by  Hoyle  (36).  He  worked  mostly  with  the 
extensor  of  the  tibia  of  the  migratory  locust  and 
showed  that  it  received  three  motor  axons.  One  of 
them,  which  ran  in  a  separate  nerve,  seemed  to  in- 
nervate all  the  fibers  of  the  muscle  and  evoked  in 
them  an  action  potential  consisting  of  a  spike  arising 
from  an  e.p.p.  The  accompanying  contraction  was  a 
rapid  twitch.  This  nerve  fiber  was  designated  by  the 
letter  F,  as  an  abbreviation  for  fast.  A  second  axon  was 
referred  to  as  Si  (signifying  a  slow  response),  even 
though  its  stimulation  resulted  in  fast  action  poten- 
tials and  contractions  in  some  of  the  muscle  fibers. 
That  is.  Si  seemed  to  have  two  different  types  of  end- 
ings and  could  produce  markedly  different  effects  in 
two  classes  of  muscle  fibers.  In  about  two-thirds  of  the 
Si-innervated  fibers,  stimulation  of  that  axon  evoked 
small,  remarkably  slow  potentials,  longer  than  i  sec. 
in  duration.  They  were  capable  of  summating  to 
plateaus  of  depolarization  of  50  mv  (during  repetitive 
nerve  stimulation)  without  giving  ri.se  to  spikes.  The 
other  one-third  of  the  fibers  supplied  by  Si  showed 
very  much  more  rapid  e.p.p.'s  which  could  give  rise 
to  spikes.  The  size  and  speed  of  these  latter  e.p.p.'s 
were  similar  to,  but  somewhat  less  than  those  follow- 
ing stimulation  of  F.  The  slow  responses  were  desig- 
nated as  Shi  and  were  accompanied  by  slow  contrac- 
tions, while  the  faster  potentials,  which  gave  rise  to 
twitches,  were  referred  to  as  Sib.  Of  the  total  number 
of  fibers  in  the  muscle,  only  about  30  per  cent  were 
supplied  with  .Si  endings  of  either  type  (20  per  cent 
Sia;  10  per  cent  Sib).  The  third  axon.  So,  which  was 
smaller  than  either  F  or  Si,  produced  an  electrical 
response  in  only  a  few  of  the  fibers  but  a  contraction 
in  apparently  none  of  them.  The  muscle  potential 
consisted  of  a  brief  depolarization  followed  by  a  more 
prolonged  hyperpolarization  (up  to  several  hundred 
msec).  Although  ijoth  phases  were  small  (less  than  i 
mv)  the  hyperpolarizations  could  summate  during 
repetitive  activity  and  thus  raise  the  resting  potential 
of  the  muscle  fiber.  The  S2  response  was  most  clearly 
seen  in  fibers  with  low  resting  potentials  and  could 
not  raise  the  membrane  potential  above  the  level  of 
about  70  mv.  Hoyle  has  not  been  able  to  demonstrate 
that  S>  causes  any  inhibition  of  either  contraction  or 
action  potentials  evoked  by  the  other  two  axons.  In 
fact,  stimulation  of  S^  sometimes  seemed  to  augment 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


247 


the  contraction  elicited  by  F,  and  Hoyle  ascrilies  to  Si 
the  function  of  raising  the  membrane  potential  briefly 
before  a  maximum  efTort  is  required  by  the  animal. 
The  unusual  features  of  the  Sl,  and  S^  responses 
would  certainly  warrant  further  study  of  the  mecha- 
nism of  their  generation.  Concerning  the  various 
combinations  of  the  responses  which  were  found  in  a 
single  muscle  fiber,  F  could  apparently  occur  with 
any  of  the  others  since  that  axon  inner\ated  all  of  the 
fibers.  Respon.ses  S2  and  Sia  were  seen  together  in  one 
fiber,  but  none  was  found  which  showed  both  Sn,  and 
So  effects. 

Aside  from  this  demonstration  of  poKneuronal  in- 
nervation, the  use  of  intracellular  electrodes  has  also 
provided  evidence  for  multiterminal  innervation. 
Working  with  the  flexor  of  the  tibia,  del  Castillo  et  al. 
(15)  found  that  cooling  the  muscle  reduced  the  size  of 
the  e.p.p.  so  that  it  did  not  give  rise  to  a  spike.  They 
then  found  that  the  height  of  the  e.p.p.  did  not  \ary 
b>-  more  than  10  to  15  per  cent  when  the  recording 
was  made  from  different  points  along  the  muscle 
fiber.  The  spikes  recorded  in  the.se  experiments  over- 
shot zero  potential  (i.e.  the  inside  of  the  fiber  became 
relatively  positive)  in  good  preparations,  but  the 
magnitude  of  the  overshoot  was  always  less  than  20 
mv  (cf.  27).  Information  was  also  obtained  concerning 
the  mechanism  of  neuromuscular  transmission.  It  was 
found  that  the  amplitude  of  the  e.p.p.  was  propor- 
tional to  the  size  of  the  resting  potential  as  experi- 
mentally altered  by  passing  current  with  a  second 
microelectrode.  This  is  the  result  which  had  previ- 
ously been  obtained  from  work  with  frog  muscle  (19) 
where  it  was  found  that  the  transmitter  released  by 
the  nerve  impulse  seemed  to  act  by  causing  an  in- 
crease in  the  conductance  of  the  endplate  membrane.' 
There  is  no  evidence  that  the  presumed  transmitter 
in  insects  is  acetylcholine.  Roeder  &  Weiant  (62)  were 
unable  to  affect  neuromuscular  transmission  with 
curarine  in  a  dilution  of  lo^''. 


MOLLUSCS 

Much  of  the  preceding  information  on  the  neuro- 
muscular mechanism  of  arthropods  was  obtainable 
because  of  several  fortunate  characteristics  of  those 

'Such  evidence  is  not  conclusive  by  itself,  howe\er.  It  has 
recently  been  shown  by  E.  Furshpan  &  D.  D  Potter  (J.  Phy- 
siol., in  press)  that  even  at  an  'electrical'  synapse  the  amplitude 
of  the  postsynaptic  response  can  vary  with  the  level  of  mem- 
brane potential. 


systems.  The  few  large  motor  axons,  which  run  in 
comparatively  long  nerves,  can  often  be  dissected  free 
and  stimulated  separately,  and  the  muscle  fibers  are 
most  often  large  and  can  be  impaled  with  micro- 
electrodes.  The  absence  of  these  features  from  most 
other  invertebrates  makes  experiments  with  them 
considerably  more  diflicult  to  perform.  Thus,  one 
finds  interesting  phenomena  which  are  difficult  to 
interpret  because  it  is  not  known  whether  they  reflect 
properties  of  the  nerves,  muscles,  neuromuscular 
junctions  or  neural  synap.ses. 

A  good  example  of  these  difficulties  is  provided  by 
a  number  of  studies  on  the  anterior  byssus  retractor 
muscle  (ABRM)  of  Mytilus  edulis.  One  of  the  more  in- 
teresting properties  of  this  and  mans-  other  lamelli- 
branch  muscles  is  the  ability  to  maintain  considerable 
tension  for  very  long  periods  of  time.  Some  muscles 
may  remain  contracted  for  more  than  ten  days  (46) 
while  durations  of  a  few  hours  are  common  (4,  37). 
Several  explanations  of  this  ability  have  been  put  for- 
ward. On  the  one  hand  there  is  the  idea  of  a  molecular 
'catch-mechanism'  (71,  77).  According  to  this  hy- 
pothesis, one  set  of  nerves  would  bring  about  a  change 
in  the  contractile  mechanism  so  that,  following  con- 
traction, the  mu.scle  could  reinain  shortened  without 
expending  additional  energy.  Another  set  of  nerves 
would  bring  about  an  active  reversal  of  this  state  and 
relax  the  muscle.  On  the  other  hand,  an  explanation 
has  been  sought  in  terms  of  already  known  properties 
of  nerve-muscle  systeins.  According  to  the  tetanus 
hypothesis,  the  muscle  would  remain  contracted 
only  as  long  as  there  were  periodic  depolarizations  of 
the  muscle  fiber  membranes,  relaxation  ensuing  at 
the  cessation  of  .such  activity  (12,  37,  46,  48).  It  has 
also  been  pointed  out  by  proponents  of  this  hypothesis 
that  the  passive  tension  decay  in  these  muscles,  follow- 
ing activation,  is  very  slow  and  that  this  factor  would 
contribute  considerably  to  the  economy  of  contrac- 
tion (2).  Molluscan  muscle  would  then  differ  from 
other  muscles  only  in  the  slowness  of  its  relaxation. 

Intermediate  between  the  'catch-mechanism'  and 
tetanus  hypothesis  is  one  in  which  the  'viscosity'  (and 
thus  the  rate  of  passive  tension  decay)  of  the  muscle 
would  be  variable,  depending  upon  the  way  in  which 
the  muscle  had  been  activated.  When  prolonged  con- 
traction was  required  the  muscle  could  be  put  into 
the  "high  viscosity'  state  and  then  infrequent  activa- 
tion would  suffice  to  maintain  a  tetanus  (39,  87).  This 
idea  was  suggested  by  Winton  (87)  following  a  study 
of  the  Mytilus  ABRM.  He  found  that  the  muscle 
responded  differently  to  alternating  and  to  direct 
current   stimulation.    Following   the   cessation   of  an 


248 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


a.c.  Stimulus  the  muscle  relaxed  comparatively 
rapidly,  but  after  d.c.  stimulation  would  do  so  very 
much  more  slowly.  In  this  slowly  relaxing  state  the 
muscle  would  support  considerable  tensions  for 
long  periods  and  seemed  to  be  less  susceptible  to 
fatigue.  In  one  example,  when  a  d.c.  stimulus  was 
applied  to  the  muscle  for  14  sec.  of  every  minute,  a 
continuous  gradually  increasing  contraction  was  ob- 
served during  the  experimental  period  (15  min.). 
When,  however,  a.c.  was  used,  also  for  14  sec.  per 
min.,  each  burst  of  stimulation  gave  rise  to  a  discrete 
contraction,  the  strength  of  which  decreased  during 
successive  minutes.  It  was  also  found  that  the  main- 
tained tension  following  a  d.c.  shock  could  be  abol- 
ished by  a.c.  stimulation.  That  is,  following  this  a.c- 
induced  contraction,  the  rate  of  relaxation  was  rapid 
even  though  the  muscle  had  been  in  the  slowly- 
relaxing  state  immediately  previously.  These  effects 
have  since  been  confirmed  by  a  number  of  authors 
(see  below).  In  a  recent  paper,  however,  Hoyle  & 
Lowy  (37)  report  that  while  they  commonly  found 
similar  results,  they  often  did  not.  In  some  case  both 
types  of  stimuli  gave  rise  to  contractions  of  equal  dura- 
tion and  sometimes  a.c.  evoked  more  prolonged 
shortening  than  did  d.c.  Nevertheless,  since  the  two 
types  of  stimulation  are  not  likely  to  occur  naturally, 
interest  lies  mainly  in  their  use  as  experimental  tools, 
and  the  dififerent  responses,  although  not  invariable, 
might  still  reflect  two  types  of  mechanisms  within  the 
muscle. 

Fletcher  (24)  confirmed  VVinton's  observations  as 
part  of  a  general  study  of  the  ABRM  in  which  he  also 
recorded  the  muscle  action  potentials.  In  most  experi- 
ments he  found  no  muscle  potentials  (after  the  initial 
one)  during  a  d.c. -induced  tonus.  This  observation 
laid  the  ground  for  the  'catch-mechanism'  hypothesis, 
van  Nieuwenhoven  (77)  also  confirmed  Winton's 
main  findings  but  was  able,  as  well,  to  duplicate  some 
of  these  effects  with  indirect  stimulation.  Relatively 
strong  faradic  stimulation  of  the  pedal  ganglion,  from 
which  the  muscle  appeared  to  receive  its  innervation, 
gave  rise  to  the  familiar  prolonged  contraction.  Subse- 
quent ganglionic  stimulation  of  lower  intensity  abol- 
ished the  remaining  tension.  Thus  he  suggested  that 
one  set  of  nerves  from  the  ganglion  would  set  the 
"catch-mechanism"  while  another  would  reverse  this 
action.  Twarog  (71)  has  recently  made  some  pharma- 
cological observations  which  she  interprets  in  terms  of 
this  same  scheme.  She  found,  for  example,  that  rela- 
tively small  concentrations  of  acetylcholine  (ACh) 
sufficed  to  give  rise  to  prolonged  contractions  of 
the    ABRM    which    were    accompanied    by    steady 


depolarization.  Washing  out  the  ACh  restored  the 
resting  potential  of  the  muscle  without,  however, 
reducing  its  tension.  This  maintained  contraction  was 
then  found  to  be  abolished  by  the  addition  of  very 
small  concentrations  of  5-hydroxytryptamine  (5-HT). 
Then  subsequent  additions  of  ACh  gave  rise  only  to 
transient  shortening  (which  could,  nevertheless,  be 
of  larger  amplitude  than  that  following  the  initial 
application  of  ACh).  Twarog  has  also  demonstrated 
the  presence  of  ACh,  a  choline  esterase  and  5-HT  in 
this  muscle.  She  suggests  that  ACh  is  the  chemical 
mediator  evoking  contraction  (and  setting  the 
'catch-mechanism')  and  that  5-HT  would  be  the 
transmitter  responsible  for  active  relaxation.  That 
these  substances  are  released  during  activity  has  not 
been  shown.  Hoyle  &  Lowy  (37)  have  confirmed  this 
inhibiting  or  relaxing  effect  of  5-HT  on  this  muscle 
but  conclude  that  it  is  not  the  natural  transmitter. 
In  their  experiments  the  contractions  were  evoked  iiy 
electrical  stimulation  rather  than  by  the  application 
of  ACh.  Before  considering  further  their  observations 
on  5-HT,  it  will  be  convenient  to  describe  another 
effect  which  they  obtained.  If  the  muscle  was  in  a 
tonic  state,  subthreshold  stimulation  would  often  bring 
about  relaxation.  It  is  likely  that  in  their  experiments 
excitation  was  effected  by  way  of  the  nerve  and  this 
observation  is  therefore  very  similar  to  that  made  by 
van  Nieuwenhoven  (see  above)  with  'weak'  shocks 
applied  to  the  ganglion.  These  authors,  however, 
refer  to  the  phenomenon  as  inhibition.  Returning 
now  to  the  effects  of  5-HT,  they  found  that  once  this 
drug  had  been  added  to  the  bath  the  prolonged, 
tonic  contractions  could  no  longer  be  evoked  by 
d.c.  stimulation,  although  the  phasic  contractions 
were  still  readily  obtainable.  The  effect  still  per- 
sisted, however,  after  several  hours,  despite  periodic 
washing  with  sea  water.  Inasmuch  as  the  inhibition 
(or  relaxation)  following  subthreshold  nerve  stimu- 
lation was  rapidly  reversible,  they  suggested  that  5-HT 
could  not  be  the  normal  mediator  of  this  effect. 

Lowy  (46,  47,  48)  and  Hoyle  &  Lowy  (37)  have 
made  a  number  of  other  observations,  all  of  which 
they  interpret  in  terms  of  the  tetanus  hypothesis.  For 
example,  they  have  almost  always  been  able,  by  using 
large  amplifications,  to  record  small  irregular  elec- 
trical activity,  presumably  muscle  potentials,  through- 
out the  duration  of  a  prolonged  contraction  in  dis- 
agreement with  Fletcher's  findings.  The  potentials 
were  localized  and  different  patterns  of  activity  were 
recorded  simultaneously  from  different  regions  of  the 
muscle.  These  potentials  were  considerably  smaller 
than  those  found  by  other  authors  in  this  muscle  (23, 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


249 


59,  70).  The  difference  would  seem  to  lie  in  the  fact 
that  on  the  one  hand  (Fletcher  and  others)  the 
synchronized  response  of  a  large  part  of  the  muscle 
fibers  was  recorded  immediately  following  the  stimu- 
lus; while  on  the  other  hand  (Hoyle  &  Lowy)  record- 
ing, at  much  higher  gain,  was  made  from  two  asyn- 
chronously active  regions  of  the  muscle  some  time 
after  cessation  of  the  stimulus.  (The  potentials  re- 
corded by  Fletcher  were  more  prolonged  than  those 
seen  by  other  workers;  but  this  might  have  been  due  to 
his  recording  apparatus.)  Nevertheless,  the  significant 
point  would  seem  to  be  that  there  is  electrical  activity 
in  the  muscle  during  the  prolonged  tonic  contractions; 
and  this  was  found  to  be  true  whether  shortening 
was  brought  about  by  d.c.  stimulation  or  addition 
of  ACh.  (The  origin  of  these  potentials,  which  persist 
for  so  long  after  a  stimulus,  will  be  considered  below.) 
.Since  these  muscles  do  .seem,  therefore,  to  require 
periodic  activation  during  tonic  contractions,  the 
main  support  for  the  catch-mechanism'  hypothesis  is 
removed.  It  will  be  recalled  that  Twarog  (71)  had 
found  that  the  muscle  could  repolarize  during  the 
tonic  contractions  induced  by  ACh.  Her  recording 
apparatus  would  not,  however,  have  detected  the 
potentials  observed  by  Lowy  and  others.  It  was  al.so 
found  by  these  latter  authors  that  when  the  muscle 
relaxed,  following  subthreshold  stimulation  or  the 
application  of  5-HT,  the  electrical  activity  ceased. 
Further,  it  has  been  found  by  Abbott  &  Lowy  (i) 
that  the  heat  production  of  the  ABRM  measured 
during  either  an  ACh-induced  tonus  or  during  a 
tetanus  (stimulating  at  2  per  sec.)  is  the  same,  al- 
though the  value  obtained  in  both  cases  is  very 
small  compared,  for  example,  to  that  of  frog  muscle. 
Thus,  the  most  attractive  interpretation  of  the  ability 
of  molluscan  muscles  to  maintain  tensions  for  pro- 
longed times  would  seem  to  be  the  one  given  by 
Abbott  &  Lowy  (2).  It  is  based  on  the  observation 
that  once  the  contractile  elements  of  these  muscles 
shorten,  they  return  to  the  rest  length  only  very  slowly, 
and  thus  infrequent  activation  suffices  for  the  main- 
tenance of  tension.  During  a  d.c. -induced  tonus  this 
repeated  activation  is  being  supplied  by  some  means, 
as  evidenced  by  the  recorded  electrical  activity. 
What  then  is  the  source  of  these  potentials,  inasmuch 
as  they  are  present  in  muscles  which  are  isolated  from 
the  central  nervous  system  for  long  times  after  the 
cessation  of  a  stimulus  and  often  in  the  apparent 
ab.sence  of  stimulation?  To  study  this  Bowden  &  Lowy 
(9)  have  examined  histologically  the  intramuscular 
nerve  supply  of  a  number  of  lamellibranch  muscles, 
including  the  ABRM.  Histochemical  methods  for  the 


detection  of  the  presence  of  cholinesterase  revealed  a 
dense  ple.xus  of  nerve  fibers  and  structures  which  they 
interpreted  to  be  nerve  cell  bodies.  These  findings 
provide  a  possible  explanation  of  the  'spontaneous' 
potentials  and  disclose  an  additional  factor  which 
must  be  considered  in  interpreting  experiments  on 
these  muscles,  namely  the  possible  presence  of 
peripheral  interneuronal  synapses.  For  example, 
Schmandt  &  Sleator  (70)  found  that  the  large  syn- 
chronous muscle  potentials  which  they  observed  in 
the  ABRM  were  conducted  decrementally  (at  a  rate 
of  about  20  cm  per  sec).  One  possible  interpretation 
of  their  results  is  that  the  muscle  fibers  show  no 
conducted  response  and  that  the  apparent  conduction 
is  carried  out  by  synapsing  intramuscular  nerve 
elements.  The  decrement  could  then  arise  from  the 
failure  of  transmission  at  some  of  these  synapses. 

The  finding  of  electrical  activity  during  the  d.c- 
induced  tonus  also  complicates  the  interpretation 
of  most  of  VVinton's  results  and  diminishes  the  neces- 
sity for  an  hypothesis  of  the  type  that  he  propo.sed. 
Nevertheless  it  is  still  possible  that  the  mu.scle  can 
relax  at  different  rates  depending  upon  the  means 
by  which  it  was  activated  (possibly  by  different  motor 
nerves). 


COELENTERATES 

As  experimental  objects  for  the  study  of  neuro 
muscular  transmission,  the  coelenterates  present  some 
of  the  same  difficulties  as  those  found  in  the  molluscs. 
The  motor  axons  are  supplied  by  a  net  of  synapsing 
neurons.  The  muscle  fibers  are  very  fine,  usually  being 
several  to  less  than  one  micron  in  diameter  when 
extended.  They  are  arranged  in  sheets  or  'fields,' 
although  there  are  places  where  the  arrangement  is 
more  compact  and  discrete  muscles  can  be  distin- 
guished. 

Aciinozoans 

Pantin  was  the  first  to  have  stimulated  these 
animals  electrically  rather  than  mechanically  and 
thus  had  some  idea  of  the  number  of  impulses  set  up 
in  the  nerve  net.  Much  of  the  work  has  been  devoted 
to  the  properties  of  this  net,  but  there  are  a  number  of 
phenomena  which  are  pertinent  here.  Most  of  the 
earlier  work  was  done  with  the  sea  anemone  Calliactis 
parasitica,  and  one  of  the  responses  studied  was  the 
contraction  of  the  sphincter  muscle  (at  the  top  of 
the  column)  following  stimuli  applied  to  the  side  of 


2^0 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  column  (54).  A  single  electric  shock  evoked  no 
visible  contraction.  Two  shocks,  however,  above  a 
certain  low  threshold  did  elicit  a  response.  The  size 
of  the  latter  was  dependent  upon  the  interval  ijetween 
the  two  stimuli,  provided  that  this  was  greater  than 
about  50  msec,  (refractory  period)  but  not  larger  than 
about  3  sees,  (facilitation  interval). 

These  phenomena  were  interpreted  ijy  Pantin  as 
follows.  The  first  of  the  two  stimuli  applied  to  the  side 
of  the  column  would  result  in  a  nerve  impulse  ar- 
riving at  each  'endplate'  (57)  of  the  sphincter  but 
would  be  unable  to  activate  the  muscle.  It  would, 
however,  facilitate  the  neuromuscular  transmission 
process  so  that  a  second  impulse  arriving  soon  after 
might  affect  activation.  The  increase  of  contraction 
size  with  shorter  intervals  would  then  be  clue  to  the 
success  of  the  nerve  impulse  at  a  greater  number  of 
junctions.  This  idea  implies  an  all-or-nothing  con- 
traction of  the  muscle  fibers.  The  advantage  of  such 
an  hypothesis  is  that  the  introduction  of  a  threshold 
for  successful  neuromuscular  transmission  helps  to 
explain  the  sharp  difTerences  between  the  effects  of 
one  and  two  stimuli. 

The  evidence  that  the  facilitation  is  a  property  of 
the  neuromuscular  junction  is  that  it  did  not  seem  to 
reside  in  either  the  nerve  net  or  the  muscle  \i\  itself. 
For  example,  a  single  impulse  traversed  the  whole 
column  nerve  net,  for  a  contraction  could  be  evoked 
by  applying  a  second  shock  with  another  pair  of 
electrodes  at  some  other  point  on  the  column,  and  a 
single  shock  could  similarly  be  shown  to  make  the 
entire  column  nerve  net  refractory.  That  the  muscle 
did  not  recjuire  facilitation  was  shown  by  applying 
the  stimuli  directly  to  the  sphincter.  Then  a  single 
shock  of  intensity  several  times  the  two-shock  threshold 
gave  rise  to  a  contraction  localized  in  the  region  of  the 
electrodes  (56).  It  seems  likely  that  stimulation  of  the 
muscle  was  direct,  since  the  shortening  which  fol- 
lowed two  weaker  shocks,  and  which  was  mediated 
by  the  nerve  net,  involved  the  whole  muscle. 

The  above  hypothesis  suggests  an  analogy  with  the 
partially  curarized  nerve-muscle  preparation  of  the 
frog  in  which  the  endplate  potentials  (e.p.p.'s) 
following  the  first  nerve  impulse  would  all  be  sub- 
threshold. Tacilitation'  would  then  represent  the 
summation  of  successive  e.p.p.'s  to  a  supraliminal 
level.  This  model  must  be  modified,  however,  to 
account  for  some  later  observations  made  by  Ross  & 
Pantin  (68).  They  found  that  if  the  interval  between 
two  stimuli  was  adjusted  so  that  the  second  shock 
just  did  not  give  rise  to  a  contraction,  then  a  third 
shock  separated  from  the  second  by  this  same  interval 


also  just  failed  to  cause  a  contraction.  On  the  basis  of 
the  abo\e  scheme,  however,  one  would  have  expected 
that  the  second  shock  would  have  brought  the  local 
excitatory  state  to  a  le\el  immediately  below  thresh- 
old. The  excitation  following  the  third  stimulus 
would  then  have  added  to  that  level  and  threshold 
would  have  been  exceeded  in  some  fibers.  To  explain 
these  oi)ser\ations  the  authors  invoked  an  extra 
process  of  sensitization  of  the  neuromuscular  junction 
which  would  be  necessary,  in  addition  to  the  excita- 
tion process.  Alternatively,  it  seems  possible  to 
account  for  these  observations  in  terms  of  a  known 
phenomenon.  This  is  the  facilitation,  as  distinct  from 
the  summation  of  e.p.p.'s,  which  occurs  at  frog 
neuromuscular  junctions  and  which  is  a  property  of 
the  ner\c  endings  (16).  That  is,  an  e.p.p.  is  not  only 
added  to  what  remains  from  a  preceding  one  but  can 
lie,  by  itself,  larger.  Thus  in  the  experiment  described 
above,  with  the  long  intervals  used,  summation  of 
local  responses  might  have  no  longer  been  present, 
and  only  the  facilitation  process  would  have  been 
operating.  It  must  be  remembered,  however,  thai 
any  electrical  concomitants  of  transmission  which 
may  Idc  present  have  not  yet  been  recorded,  and  such 
attempted  explanations  are  only  speculative. 

The  responses  of  the  sphincter  of  CaUiaclis  described 
above  are  very  similar  to  those  recorded  from  the 
longitudinal  retractors  of  the  mesenteries  of  Mi-tridmrn 
senile  (28).  In  both  species  these  muscles  bring  about 
withdrawal  responses  and  provide  the  quickest  con- 
tractions of  which  the  animal  is  capable.  They  may 
be  likened  to  the  escape  reactions  of  some  of  the 
higher  invertebrates  (earthworm,  squid  and  crayfish) 
which  are  mediated  by  giant  nerve  fibers.  It  is 
apparent,  however,  that  all  the  other  activities  of  the 
anemone,  such  as  locomotion  and  feeding,  are  built 
up  from  \ery  slow  contractions  (5,  28,  55).  The  latter 
include  the  slowest  contraction  known,  and  are  so 
leisurely  that  usually  no  movement  can  be  seen  on 
casual  oijservation,  despite  the  fact  that  very  consider- 
able changes  in  the  shape  of  the  animal  are  almost 
continuously  taking  place  (as  shown  by  time-lapse 
photography,  etc.). 

Although  the  fundamental  difference  between  the 
fast  and  slow  contractions  is  not  known,  an  operational 
definition  of  the  two  is  supplied  by  the  following 
criteria  assembled  from  Batham  &  Pantin  (6)  and 
Ross  (67).  It  seems  particularh  useful  to  distinguish 
two  separate  contraction  types  since  both  occur  in  the 
same  muscles,  a)  The  slow  contractions  are  evoked  by 
lower  frequencies  of  stimulation.  Whereas  one  shock 
everv  three  seconds  is  usually  about  the  lowest  fre- 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


-a' 


quenc\'  for  evoking  fast  contractions,  slow  ones  persist 
with  shock  intervals  up  to  about  15  sec.  Apparently, 
frequencies  above  one  or  two  stimuli  per  second  do  not 
succeed,  and  there  is  an  optimal  frequency  which  may 
be  considerably  lower  than  this.  For  example,  in  one 
experiment  (67)  a  small  slow  contraction  superim- 
posed on  the  quick  one  followed  five  shocks  separated 
by  intervals  of  1.2  sec.  The  maximal  slow  contraction, 
however,  in  response  to  this  number  of  stimuli  was 
not  obtained  until  the  intervals  between  them  were 
increased  to  13.6  sec.  b)  Whereas  the  fast  contraction 
ensues  within  less  than  o.  i  sec.  after  the  first  effective 
stimulus  (usually  the  second  shock),  the  slow  contrac- 
tion may  not  begin  until  30  to  1 50  sec.  after  the 
beginning  of  a  train  of  stimuli,  c)  Five  or  six  stimuli, 
rather  than  two,  are  often  the  fewest  that  will  evoke 
a  visible  .slow  response.  The  size  of  the  contraction 
increases  with  additional  shocks  up  to  a  maximum. 
(f)  The  rising  phase  of  a  summated  fast  contraction 
has  the  appearance  of  an  incomplete  tetanus.  The 
rise  time  for  each  step  usually  occupies  less  than  i  sec. 
The  slow  contraction,  however,  is  entirely  smooth, 
and  the  rising  phase  may  extend  over  0.5  to  i  min. 
The  initial  rate  of  rise  is  extremely  slow. 

The  fast  and  slow  contractions  ha\e  been  shown  to 
occur  in  some  of  the  same  muscles,  such  as  the 
sphincters  of  Calliactis  and  Alelridium,  the  longitudinal 
retractors  of  Metridium,  etc.  Can,  then,  a  single  muscle 
fiber  contract  in  both  ways?  There  is  some  indirect 
evidence  that  this  can  happen.  When  the  slow 
contraction  of  muscles,  capable  of  also  giving  strong 
fast  responses,  is  observed  under  the  microscope,  all 
regions  of  the  muscle  can  be  seen  to  be  shortening  and 
no  local  buckling  occurs  (6).  If  the  recosery  of  tension 
following  a  quick  release  is  compared  during  the  two 
types  of  response  in  the  same  muscle,  it  is  found  that 
the  time  course  is  rapid  in  both  cases  and  similar  to  the 
original  rate  of  tension  development  for  the  fast 
contraction  (67).  Thus,  there  is  some  reason  to  be- 
lieve that  the  same  contractile  material  gives  rise  to 
both  contractions,  and  that  the  rate  and  extent  of 
activation  of  the  contractile  substance  is  the  distin- 
guishing feature.  One  is  then  faced  with  the  problem 
of  how  the  two  different  types  of  activation  are 
brought  about.  The  fact  that  the  slow  shortening 
exhibits  a  longer  refractory  period  (i.e.  has  a  higher 
minimum  effective  stimulation  frequency)  than  the 
fast  contraction  suggests  that  different  excitable  ele- 
ments are  involved.  But  if  the  same  muscle  fibers  give 
both  types  of  shortening,  these  elements  must  be  the 
nerve  fibers,  and  thus  one  might  expect  to  find  more 
than  one  nerve  net  innervating  such  muscles.  W'hile 


there  is  good  histological  evidence  in  the  scyphozoans 
(see  below)  for  distinct  nerve  nets  mediating  different 
contractions,  no  such  observations  have  been  made  in 
the  actinians,  and  there  is,  as  yet,  too  little  information 
to  resolve  this  question. 

Scyphozoans 

Associated  with  their  more  free-living  existence,  the 
behavior  patterns  of  the  medusae  may  differ  con- 
siderably from  those  of  the  anemones.  Their  most 
conspicuous  activity  is  a  comparatively  rapid,  more 
or  less  rhythmical,  contraction  of  the  circular  muscu- 
lature of  the  bell.  These  contractions  provide  the 
basic  swimming  movement.  Bullock  (13)  has  studied 
them  using  strip  preparations  (63)  from  three  species 
of  scyphozoans  and  has  compared  them  with  the 
contractions  of  anemone  muscles.  They  differ  from  the 
quick  contractions  of  the  mesenteric  retractors  of 
Metridium  (see  above)  in  several  respects,  a)  A  single 
threshold  shock  usually  evokes  some  contraction  of 
the  bell,  b)  The  duration  of  the  facilitation  interval  in 
the  bell  is  longer  than  in  the  retractor.  In  the 
former  muscle,  a  contraction  following  a  previous 
one  by  about  seven  .seconds  is  usually  still  augmented. 
c)  The  duration  of  a  single  contraction  is  a  fraction 
of  that  in  the  medusa  preparation.  (/)  The  absolute 
refractory  period  of  the  bell  musculature  is  several 
times  longer  (about  700  msec,  in  the  medusae  as 
compared  with  probably  less  than  200  msec,  in  the 
anemones). 

Because  of  the  long  refractory  period  and  the 
relatively  short  duration  of  the  mechanical  event, 
there  can  be  very  little  summation  of  tension  during  a 
series  of  contractions  of  the  bell;  and  facilitation 
appears  as  an  increase  in  the  strength  of  separate 
successive  'twitches.'  In  the  anemone  mesenteric 
retractor,  on  the  other  hand,  the  facilitation  interval 
is  shorter  than  the  duration  of  the  contraction  and 
facilitation  and  summation  are  always  seen  together. 
These  differences  can  be  related  to  the  differences  in 
function  of  the  two  types  of  muscles.  The  anemone 
retractors  are  involved  in  withdrawal  respon.ses  and, 
with  summation  of  successive  contractions,  can  bring 
about  a  striking  decrease  in  the  height  of  the  animal. 
These  muscles  can  shorten  to  less  than  20  per  cent  of 
their  extended  length.  The  musculature  of  the 
medusan  bell,  by  contrast,  pro\ides  a  pumping  action 
and  resembles  the  vertebrate  heart  in  having  a 
relatively  long  refractory  period. 

The  site  of  the  facilitation  is  probably,  by  analogy 
with  the  actinians,   the   neuromuscular  jimction.    It 


252 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


is  not  a  property  of  the  nerve  net,  for,  following  a 
single  shock,  the  excitation  spreads  over  the  entire 
bell  (13);  Horridge  (31)  has  shown  that  this  excitation 
consists  of  a  single  all-or-nothing  nerve  impulse.  It 
has  not  been  proved,  however,  that  the  facilitation  is 
not  a  property  of  the  excitation-contraction  coupling 
within  the  muscle  fibers. 

There  have  been  several  histological  studies  of  the 
nerve  net  which  presumably  distributes  the  excitation 
of  the  swimming  nio\ement.  Schafer  (69)  described 
large  bipolar  neurons  in  the  subumbrellar  epithelium. 
The  nerve  fibers  were  more  or  less  straight,  were 
usually  unbranched  and  were  comparatively  thick 
(15  /i).  This  description  has  recently  been  confirmed 
by  Horridge  (32)  who  found  that  the  nerve  fibers  were 
in  the  range  6  to  12  n.  Both  authors  described  tapering 
of  the  nerve  fibers  towards  their  ends,  but  there  was 
no  consistently  observed  structure  which  would 
constitute  a  motor  nerve  ending  [see,  however,  the 
illustrations  in  Schafer  (69)].  Bozler  (11)  also 
described  the  large  bipolar  neurons,  and  observed 
smaller  ones,  as  well  as  numerous  multipolar  neurons 
with  branching  axons.  One  might  then  wonder  if  the 
different  types  of  neurons  form  physiologically 
distinct  nerve  nets  which  would  underlie  different 
behavioral  responses  (i.e.  types  of  contraction).  This 
question  will  be  con.sidered  next. 

Aside  from  the  swimming  movements  described 
above,  localized  and  more  prolonged  contractions  of 
regions  of  the  bell  can  also  be  observed  (10,  13,  33). 
Since  it  has  been  shown  that  the  excitation  underlying 
the  swimming  movement  is  spread  over  the  entire 
bell  by  a  single  impulse  traversing  a  nerve  net  (31), 
the  presence  of  local  contractions  does  suggest  the 
existence  of  another  nerve  net  in  which  the  spread  of 
excitation  is  limited.  Further,  Romanes  (63)  showed 
that  a  wave  of  excitation,  distinct  from  the  wave  of 
contraction,  could  cross  the  bell.  In  one  demonstra- 
tion of  this,  he  removed  seven  of  the  eight  marginal 
ganglia  (in  which  the  e.xcitation  for  the  rhythmical 
swimming  movements  arise)  and  applied  to  some 
point  on  the  bell  a  stimulus  too  weak  to  evoke  a 
contraction  wave  (swimming  movement)  directly. 
Then  after  some  delay,  a  contraction  wave  would 
spread  out  from  the  intact  ganglion.  Additional 
examples  suggesting  the  presence  of  more  than  one 
nerve  net  can  be  found  in  Horridge  (33). 

One  more  case,  however,  will  be  considered  for 
here  there  is  good  correlation  between  histological 
and  physiological  observations.  Horridge  (34)  has 
studied  the  ephyra  larva  of  Auirllia  which  shows  two 
types  of  contractions.  There  are  a)  the  generalized. 


rapid,  rhythmic  swimming  movements  and  A)  pro- 
longed contractions  involving  a  variable  fraction  of  the 
animal  and  normally  associated  with  feeding.  Follow- 
ing strong  mechanical  stimulation,  the  prolonged 
contraction  may  involve  the  whole  animal.  In  histo- 
logical preparations,  two  types  of  nerve  net  can  be 
distinguished.  The  first  is  composed  of  bipolar  neurons 
which  are  confined  almost  entirely  to  the  epithelium 
overlying  the  radial  and  circular  musculature.  The 
fibers  of  these  neurons  are  highly  oriented,  running 
in  the  same  direction  as  the  muscle  fibers.  The  second 
net  consists  mainly  of  multipolar  cells,  with  some 
bipolars,  and  is  present  throughout  the  entire  epithe- 
lium. The  fibers  of  this  net  are  not  particularly 
oriented  except  in  the  region  around  the  mouth.  Some 
of  the  neurons  of  this  diffuse  net  send  fibers  to  the 
surface  of  the  epithelium  and  would  appear  to  be 
sensory  cells.  This  observation  alone  suggests  that  the 
diffuse  net  is  associated  with  the  local,  prolonged 
contractions  since  these  latter  are  evoked  by  tactile 
stimuli.  Experiments  designed  to  test  this  hypothesis 
yielded  affirmative  results  and  also  provided  evidence 
that  the  swimming  movements  are  mediated  by  the 
other  net  of  bipolar  oriented  neurons.  For  example, 
eight  radial  cuts  were  made  through  the  disc  so  that 
the  band  of  circular  muscle,  with  its  overlying  net  of 
bipolar  cells,  was  sectioned  into  eight  separate  arcs. 
The  cuts  were  not  continued  all  the  way  to  the  center 
of  the  disc  and  the  animal  thus  remained  in  one  piece. 
It  was  then  found  that  each  arm,  with  its  arc  of  circu- 
lar muscle,  still  produced  the  rhythmical  swimming 
movements  but  that  the  beat  of  each  was  independent 
of  all  the  others.  A  strong  tactile  stimulus,  however, 
could  still  produce  a  co-ordinated  contraction  of  the 
whole  animal. 

The  Mechanism  oj  Transmission  ni  Coelenterates 

Practically  nothing  is  known  of  the  actual  mech- 
anism by  which  nerve-net  excitation  crosses  the 
junction  to  the  muscle  fibers.  Tissue  extracts  have 
been  made  and  tested  on  neuromuscular  transmission 
but  without  success  (65,  66).  Numerous  drugs  have 
been  tested  on  the  responses  ot  the  sphincter  muscle 
of  intact  Calliaclis.  Acetylcholine,  curare,  nicotine, 
epinephrine,  histamine  and  a  number  of  other  drugs 
are  without  apparent  effect  (64).  Several  drugs, 
however,  were  effective  in  high  concentrations  and 
after  prolonged  exposure.  Tyramine,  tryptamine  and 
933F,  after  immersion  of  the  animal  in  solutions  of 
io~*  gm  per  ml  for  one-  and  one-half  to  several  hours, 
brought  about  se\eral-fold  increases  in  the  muscular 


NEUROMUSCULAR    TRANSMISSION     IN    INVERTEBRATES 


253 


response,  and  contractions  following  single  shocks 
were  then  commonly  seen.  These  drugs  did  not  evoke 
any  contraction  in  the  absence  of  other  stimuli. 
Depressant  drugs  were  also  found.  Ergotoxin  (io^°) 
and  trimethylamine  oxide  (io~'),  respectively,  re- 
duced and  abolished  sphincter  contraction.  The  fact 
that  such  high  concentrations  and  long  application 
times  were  required  need  not  argue  against  the  signifi- 
cance of  the.se  results,  since  there  are  probably  con- 
siderable barriers  to  diffusion  in  the  intact  animal, 
and  all  effects  observed  were  full)-  reversible.  Magne- 


sium chloride  also  depresses  neuromuscular  transmis- 
sion both  in  anemones  (68)  and  in  medusae  (13)  and 
in  these  cases  high  concentrations  and  prolonged 
exposures  are  also  necessary. 

While  the  above  experiments  may  provide  interest- 
ing clues,  they  do  not  allow  any  specific  conclusions 
about  the  mechanism  of  transmission,  and  there  would 
not  seem  to  be  any  a  prion  basis  on  which  to  decide 
whether  this  process  occurs  in  these  animals  by  local 
circuit  action  ('electrically')  or  by  means  of  some 
mediator  ('chemically'). 


REFERENCES 


9- 
10. 
1 1. 
12. 
'3- 
14- 

15- 
16. 


18. 

19- 
20. 
21. 
22. 

23- 

24. 

25- 
26. 

27- 


29- 
30. 
3'- 
32. 
33 
34- 


.\bbott,  B.  C.  and  J.  Lowv.  J.  P/nyiol.  130:  25P,  1955. 
.Abbott,  B.  C.  and  J.  Lowv.  J.  mat.  bin!.  A.  V.  A'.  35:  521, 

'956. 

Bacq,  Z.  M.  Arch,  internal,  physiol.  42:  47,  1935. 

Barnes,  G.  E.  J.  E.xper.  Biol.  32:  158,  1955. 

Batham,   E.  J.   AND  C.   F.  A.   Pantin    J.  E.xper.   Biol.  27: 

290.  1950. 

Batham,  E.  J.  AND  C.  F.  A.  Pantin.  J.  E.xper.  Biol.  31 :  84, 

1954- 

BiEDERMANN,  W.  Sitzbir.  Al.ad.   Wis.i.   Wien.  QAbt.  III.')  96: 

8;  1887. 

BiEDERM.\NN,    W.    Elechophysiologv.    London:    Macinillan. 

1898,  vol.  IL 

BowDEN,  J.  AND  J.  Lowv.  .Nature,  London  176:  346,  1955. 

BozLER,  E.  Z^schr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  4:  797,  1926. 

BozLER,  E.  ^tschr.  ^ellforsch.  u.  mikroskop.  Anal.  5:  244,  1927. 

BozLER,  E.  Experientia  4:  213,  1948. 

Bullock,  T.  H.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  22:  251,  1943. 

d'Ancona,    U.    Trab.    Lab.   Invest.    Biol.    Univ.   Madrid.    23: 

393.  1925- 

DEL  Castillo,  J.,  G.  Hovle  .and  X.  M.\chne.  J.  Physiol. 

121:  539-  '953- 

del  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  124:  574,  1954. 
Ellis,  C.  H.,  C.  H.  Thienes  and  C.  A.  G.  Wiersm.^.  Biol. 
Bull.  83:  334,  1942. 
Fatt,  p.  Physiol.  Rev.  34:  674,   1954. 
Fatt,  p.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  115:  320,  1951. 
Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  120:  171,  1 953. 
Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  121 :  374,  1953. 
Fatt,  P.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  30:  433,  1953. 
Fletcher,  C.  M.  J.  Physiol.  90:  233,  1937. 
Fletcher,  C.  M.  J.  Physiol,  go:  415,  1937. 
Foettincer,  a.  Arch.  Biol.  Paris  i  :  279,  1880. 
Furshpan,  E.  J.  Doctors  Thesis.   Pasadena,  Calif.:  Cali- 
fornia Inst,  of  Technology,  1955. 
Hagiw.\ra,  S.  and  .a.  Watanabe.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  4:  65, 

1954- 

Hall,  D.  M.  and  C.  F.  A.  Pantin.  J.  Exper.  Binl.  14:  71, 

■937- 

Hoffmann,  P.  ^tschr.  Biol.  63:  411,  1914. 
Holmes,  W.  .Nature,  London  151 :  531,  1943. 
HoRRiDGE,  G.  A.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  31  :  594,  1954. 
HrjRRIDGE,  G.  .\.  Quart.  J.  Microsc.  Sc.  95:  85,  1954. 
HoRRiDGE,  G.  A.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  33 :  366,  1 956. 
HoRRiDGE,  G.  A.  Quart.  J.  Microsc.  Sc.  97:  59,  1956. 


35.  Hovle,  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  143:  281,  1955. 

36.  Hovle,  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  143:  343,  1955. 

37.  Hoyle,  G.  and  J.  Lowv.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  33:  295,  1956. 

38.  Hunt,  C.  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J.  Physiol.  126:  293,  1954. 

39.  Johnson,  W.  H.  Biol.  Bull.  107:  326,  1955. 

40.  Katz,  B.  J.  Physiol.  87:  199,  1936. 

41.  Katz,  B.  Biol.  Rev.  24:  i,  1949. 

42.  Katz,  B.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B 

133:  374.  1946- 

43.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  Neurophysiol.  17:  558,  1954. 

44.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Neurophysiol .  9:  337,  1946. 

45.  Kuffler,    S.    W.    and    E.    M.    Vaughan   Williams.    J. 
Physiol.   121:  289,  1953. 

46.  Lowv,  J.  J'.  Physiol.   120:   129,   1953. 

47.  LowY,  J.  J.  Physiol.  124:  100,  1954. 

48.  LowY,  J.  Nature,  London  176:  345,  1955. 

49.  Lucas,  K.  J.  Physiol.  35:  326,  1907. 

50.  Lucas,  K.  J.  Physiol.  51 :  i,  1917. 

51.  Mangold,  E.  ^tschr.  allg.  Physiol.  5:  135,  1905. 

52.  Marcu,  O.  Anat.  Anz-  67:  369,  1929. 

53.  Marmont,  G.  and  C.  a.  G.  Wiersma.  J.  Physiol.  93:  173, 
1938. 

54.  Pantin,  C.  F.  A.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  12:  119,  1935. 

55.  Pantin,  C.  F.  A.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  12;  139,  1935. 

56.  Pantin,  C.  F.  A.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  12;  389,  1935. 

57.  Pantin,  C.  F.  A.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  140:  147,  1952. 

58.  Prinole,  J.  W.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  16:  220,  1939. 

59.  Prosser,  C.  L.,  H.  J.  Curtis  and  D.  M.  Travis.  J.  Cell.  & 
Comp.  Physiol.  38:  299,  1951. 

60.  Richet,  C.  Physiologic  des  Muscles  el  des  Nerjs.  Paris:  Balliere, 
1882. 

61.  RiPLEv,  S.  H   Doctor's  Thesis.  Pasadena,  Calif:  California 
Inst,  of  Technology,  1953. 

62.  RoEDER,  K.  D.  and  E.  a.  Weiant.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  27:  i, 
1950. 

63.  Romanes,  G.  J.  Philos.  Trans.  167(1):  659,  1878. 

64.  Ross,  D.  M.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  22:  21,  1945. 

65.  Ross,  D.  M.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  22;  32,  1945. 

66.  Ross,  D.  M.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  29:  235,  1952. 

67.  Ross,  D.  M.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  34:  11,  1957. 

68.  Ross,  D.  M.  AND  C.  F.  A.  Pantin.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  17:  61, 
1940. 

69.  ScHAFER,  E.  A.  Philos.  Trans.  169(11):  563,  1879. 

70.  ScHMANDT,   W.   AND   W.    Sleator,  Jr.   J.    Cell.  &   Comp 
Physiol.  46:439,  1955. 


254 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


71.  TwAROG,  B.  M.  J   Cell.  c£?  Comp.  Physiol.  44:  141,  1954. 

72.  VAN  Harreveld,  a.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  16:  398,  1939. 

73.  van  Harreveld,  A.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  70:  267,  1939. 

74    van  Harreveld,  A.   and  C.   A.   G.  Wiersma.  J.  Physiol. 
88:  78,  1936. 

75.  van   Harreveld,   A.   and   C.   A.    G.   Wiersma.   J.   E.xper. 
Biol.  14:448,  1937- 

76.  van   Harreveld,   A.   and   C.   A.   G.   Wiersma.   J.   E.xper. 
Bwl.  16:  121,  1939. 

77.  van    Nieuwenhoven,    L.    M.    Doctor's    Thesis.    Utrecht: 
State  Univ.  Utrecht,  1947. 

78.  Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  74:  63,  1941. 

79.  Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  Biol.  Symp.  3:  259,  1941. 

80.  Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  Arch,  neerl.  ^ool.  11:1,  1955. 


81.  Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  and  C.  H.  Ellis,  J.  Exper.  Btol.   18: 
223,  1942. 

82.  Wiersma,   C.   A.   G.   and  A.   van   Harreveld.  J.   Exper. 
Biol.  15:  18,  1938. 

83.  Wiersma,  C.  .A.  G.  and  .\.  van  Herreveld    Phvsiol.  .^nol. 
'i:  75.  1938. 

84.  Wiersma,  C.  .\.  G.  and  .\.  van  Herreveld.  Physiol,  ^nol. 

12:  43.  1939- 

85.  Wiersma,  C.  A.  G.  and  S.  H.  Ripley.  Physiol,  comparata  el 
Oecol.  2:  391,  1952. 

86.  Wilson,  V.  J.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  31  :  280,  1954. 

87.  Winton,  F.  R.  J.  Physiol.  88:  492,  1937. 

88.  Wright,  E    B.  J.  Cell.  Ci  Comp.  Physiol.  33:  301,  1949. 


CHAPTER    IX 


Brain  potentials  and  rhythms — introduction 


A.  FES  SARD  College  de  France,  Paris,  France 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

General  Nature  of  Brain  Potentials 
Special  Characteristics  of  Brain  Potentials 
Functional  Significance  of  Brain  Potentials 
Microphysiological  Studies 
Macrophysiological  Studies 


but  an  introduction  to  this  chapter  may  better  take 
into  account  more  fundamental  features  of  this  field 
of  research  by  considering  broadly  the  three  questions 
of  the  general  nature,  the  special  characteristics  and 
the  functional  significance  of  brain  potentials  and 
rhythms,  each  special  aspect  being  dealt  with  in  de- 
tail in  subsequent  chapters. 


FOR  OBVIOUS  REASONS,  the  stuclv  of  brain  potentials 
and  of  their  rhythms  is  by  far  the  most  complicated 
task  that  has  ever  been  proposed  to  electrophysi- 
ologists.  It  is  therefore  not  surprising  that  its  develop- 
ment has  been  relatively  slow.  In  the  period  from  1875 
to  1913,  the  names  of  Caton  C'S),  Fleischl 
von  Marxow  (25),  Danilewski  (19),  Beck  &  Cybulski 
(7)  and  Prawdicz-Neminsky  (44,  45)  stand  out  among 
the  few  pioneers  who  experimented  on  animals, 
preceding  the  epoch-making  discovery  of  brain  waves 
in  man  by  Hans  Berger  in  1924  [first  published  in 
1929  (8)].  As  a  matter  of  fact,  not  before  the  end  of 
the  first  third  of  this  century  did  cerebral  electro- 
physiology  truly  enter  the  regular  scope  of  neuro- 
physiological  research  with  the  first  works  of  Fischer 
(24),  Kornmuller  (35,  36),  Bartley  (6),  Bishop  (9), 
Adrian  &  Matthews  (2),  Gerard  et  at.  (26,  27),  Wang 
C50),  Bremer  (10),  Gozzano  (29),  Adrian  (i)  and 
Jasper  (31).  During  this  same  period,  clinical  elec- 
troencephalography was  developing  vigorously  and 
furnishing  itself  results  important  for  the  compre- 
hension of  cerebral  mechanisms.  Perhaps  in  no  other 
field  of  neurophysiology  is  there  such  a  reciprocity 
of  relations  between  the  findings  of  investigation  on 
experimental  animals  and  those  of  clinical  observa- 
tions. This  circumstance  has  contributed  to  the  par- 
ticular character  of  cerebral  electrophysiology  today; 


GENER.'VL  N.4TURE   OF  BR.AIN   POTENTI.^LS 

The  question  of  the  nature  of  brain  potentials 
leads  us  back  to  the  preceding  chapters  on  neuron 
physiology,  for  there  is  nothing  essentially  new  appear- 
ing in  the  ijrain  of  a  bioelectrical  nature,  nothing 
not  having  a  physicochcmical  basis  common  to  all 
neurons.  The  fundamental  phenomena  of  neuronal 
activity,  i.e.  brief  all-or-nothing  spikes,  graded  slow 
waves,  potential  gradients  of  ill-defined  duration, 
have  been  well  recognized  as  the  sole  components  of 
brain  potentials.  On  the  other  hand,  conduction  of 
impulses  along  fibers,  transmission  of  excitation  or 
inhibition  across  synapses,  electrotonic  spread  and 
"ephaptic'  interactions,  and  finally  rhythmic  genera- 
tion of  potentials  are  the  general  kinetic  operations 
from  which  all  attempts  to  explain  the  integrated  ac- 
tivities of  the  brain  must  start. 

Detection  of  these  elementary  processes  within  the 
brain  and  description  of  their  quantitative  parameters 
as  compared  to  those  of  neurons  belonging  to  other 
structm-es  (such  as  the  spinal  cord,  ganglia  in  verte- 
brates or  invertebrates,  peripheral  sensory  neurons) 
are  the  tasks  that  have  been  and  are  still  being  carried 
out  by  electrophysiologists  since  the  pioneer  work  of 
Renshaw  et  al.  (46)  who  introduced  the  highly  re- 
warding microelectrode  technique  in  brain  physiology. 


255 


■2.=s6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


Among  those  who  early  worked  along;  this  line,  let  us 
mention  Moruzzi  and  several  collaborators  of  his 
school  (42  and  many  subsequent  papers),  Jung  et  al. 
(34),  Amassian  (5),  Li  &  Jasper  (38),  Albe-Fessard 
&  Buser  (3,  4),  Tasaki  and  colleagues  (49),  Rose  & 
Mountcastle  (48)  and  Phillips  (43).  These  workers 
have  started  a  probably  long-lasting  and  fruitful  era 
of  intensive  microexploration  of  cerebral  structures. 
For  the  identification  and  analysis  of  single  unit  ac- 
tivities within  the  brain,  knowledge  already  acquired 
from  more  accessible  structures,  particularly  from 
spinal  motoneurons,  can  be  of  great  help  (see  Chapters 
III  and  X  of  this  work). 

In  another  direction,  leading  down  to  the  molecular 
le\el,  are  the  investigations  of  those  interested  in  bio- 
physical and  biochemical  mechanisms,  as  well  as 
drug  actions,  that  are  related  to,  or  interfere  with, 
electrical  activities  of  brain  tissue.  The  way  in  which 
these  activities  depend  upon  metabolic  factors,  circu- 
latory and  respiratory  conditions,  ionic  and  hormonal 
content  of  the  milieu  interieur,  is  far  from  being  exactly 
known.  An  alteration  of  the  resting  potentials  is 
assumed  to  be  the  basis  of  some  ionic  and  drug  actions. 


SPECI.\L  CH.'^RACTERISTICS  OF  BRAIN  POTENTIALS 

Brain  potentials,  apart  from  the  common  aspects 
they  share  with  other  biopotentials,  have  special 
characteristics  which  are  related  to  the  structure  and 
specific  properties  of  the  tissue  within  which  they  are 
engendered.  How  these  relations  can  explain  the 
different  modalities  of  potentials  encountered  is  the 
central  theme  of  the  chapters  constituting  the  present 
subsection  of  this  volume.  A  bread  survey  of  the 
factors  involved  may  help  to  grasp  the  wonderful 
complexity  and  dixersity  of  electrical  manifestations 
offered  by  a  mammalian  brain,  either  in  its  .so-called 
spontaneous  activity  or  under  experimental  condi- 
tions including  controlled  stimulation.  Thus  we  come 
to  the  classical  distinction  between  evoked  potentials 
(^considered  by  Chang  in  Chapter  XII)  and  autogenic 
rhythms  (discussed  by  Walter  in  Chapter  XI)  to 
which  a  transitional  modality  must  be  added,  that  of 
induced  rhythmical  activities  of  temporary  character 
or  after-discharges  (also  appearing  in  the  chapter 
by  Chang). 

Other  general  distinctions  within  the  field  of  brain 
potentials  will  be  considered  below,  together  with 
some  of  the  problems  confronted  by  the  modern 
neurophysiologist. 


FUNCTIONAL    SIGNIFICANCE    OF    BRAIN    POTENTIALS 

The  third  aspect  is  the  functional  significance  of 
brain  potentials  and  their  rhythms.  We  are  not  di- 
rectly concerned  here  with  this  functional  aspect 
which  will  be  examined  in  later  chapters  of  this 
volume.  However,  it  is  ditticult  and  to  a  certain 
extent  artificial,  once  a  potential  has  been  described, 
not  to  speak  of  the  link  it  appears  to  have  with  an 
actual  operation  of  the  nervous  system  of  which  it 
thus  becomes  a  sign:  projection  of  an  afferent  message, 
interactions  between  central  activities  or  emission  of 
efferent  impulses.  This  most  often  invokes  simple 
questions  of  functional  topography  or  chronology 
but  may  also  go  so  far  as  to  relate  to  highly  integrated 
psychological  processes  (as  will  appear  in  Walter's 
chapter)  or  to  the  well-defined  symptoms  of  patho- 
logical behavior  such  as  those  of  epileptic  seizures 
(described  by  Gastaut  in  Chapter  XIV)  once  it  has 
been  recognized  that  reliable  correlations  exist  be- 
tween these  phenomena  and  some  parameter  or 
parameters  of  brain  potentials  or  rhythms  that  have 
initially  been  studied  for  themselves.  New  specific 
aspects  of  brain  potentials  are  often  discovered  as  a 
consequence  of  functional  explorations  of  this  kind. 

Let  us  return  to  the  special  characteristics  of  po- 
tentials arising  within  the  brain.  These  appear  either 
in  the  form  of  more  or  less  durable  states — potential 
gradients  and  regular  periodic  changes — or  in  the 
form  of  responses  to  direct  or  indirect  stimuli.  In 
l)oth  cases,  one  must  clearly  distinguish  the  micro- 
physiological  approach  applying  to  single  units  from 
the  record  of  potentials  arising  within  more  or  less 
numerous  neuronal   populations. 


.\IlCROPHYSIOLOGIC.\L    STUDIES 

The  microphysiological  approach  reveals  not  only 
the  most  common  processes  of  neural  electrogenesis 
but  al.>;o  important  differences  between  the  elec- 
trical behavior  of  single  units.  Among  the  various 
types  of  neurons,  some  are  more  accessible  than  others 
to  microelectrode  study.  The  pyramidal  cells  of  the 
cerebral  cortex,  the  Purkinje  cells  of  the  cerebellum, 
the  neurons  of  the  main  sensory  relay  nuclei  and  those 
in  the  midbrain  reticular  formation  and  the  centrum 
medianum  of  the  thalamus  ha\c  l:)een  the  most  care- 
fully studied.  The  general  shape  of  the  neuron,  the 
distribution    of  synapses   along   its   surface   and    the 


BRAIN    POTENTIALS    AND    RHYTHMS INTRODUCTION 


257 


differential  properties  of  its  successive  segments — the 
dendrites,  cell  body,  axon  hillock,  myelinated  axon, 
naked  branches  and  endings — are  determining  factors 
of  its  electrical  behavior.  One  of  the  more  important 
contributions  of  contemporary  research  is  the  un- 
veiling of  the  distinctive  electrogenic  properties  of 
dendrites  at  least  in  certain  specialized  neurons  ijy 
studies  such  as  those  of  Buser  (15),  Chang  (i  7),  Clare 
&  Bishop  (18),  Grundfest  &  Purpura  (30),  Lorente 
de  No  (41)  and  Roitbak  (47).  Dependency  of  recorded 
potentials  upon  morphological  characteristics  of  the 
neurons  was  initially  recognized  by  Lorente  de  No 
(40)  when  he  made  a  distinction  between  neurons 
generating  open  fields  and  those  generating  closed  or 
semiclosed  fields. 

The  problems  attacked  by  the  microphysiological 
technique  involve  the  most  fundamental  operations 
taking  place  in  the  brain,  the  neuron  being  considered 
as  a  relay,  as  a  focus  of  integration  or  as  a  source  of 
rhythmic  activity.  All  three  cases  pose  the  common 
question  of  the  way  in  which  slow  waves — i.e.  post- 
synaptic potentials,  after-potentials,  autogenic  local 
variations  of  the  resting  potential — and  spikes  or 
trains  of  spikes  interact  with  each  other. 

Bombardinent  by  afferent  impulses  leads  to  the 
build-up  of  slow  variations,  negative  in  the  case  of 
excitation  and  po.sitive  with  inhibition.  These  slow 
waves  in  turn  produce,  accelerate,  slow  or  suppress 
efferent  impulses.  Through  these  two  closely  allied 
reciprocal  processes,  the  neuron  performs  its  ele- 
mentary functions.  From  this  rather  monotonous 
theme  of  action,  almost  infinite  varieties  of  neural 
behavior  arise,  determined  partly  by  the  intrinsic 
properties  of  the  neuron  and  partly  by  those  of  its 
environment,  including  its  connections  with  other 
neurons. 

This  last  consideration  draws  attention  to  the  notion 
that  in  the  central  nervous  system,  and  especially  in 
the  brain,  unit  activity  described  in  isolation  would 
be  nonsense.  Simultaneous  recordings  from  single 
units  in  different  parts  of  the  brain  with  a  inultitude 
of  microelectrodes  is  a  technical  achievement  that 
cannot  go  very  far  relatively  to  the  number  of  neurons 
involved  in  the  simplest  operations  carried  out  by  the 
cerebral  structures.  This  brings  us  to  examine  the 
resources  of  the  macrophysiological  approach. 


MACROPHYSIOLOGICAL  STUDIES 

A  priori,  the  macrophysiological  approach  can  give 
significant  results  only  when  a  large  mass  of  neurons 


working  in  approximate  synchrony  is  activated. 
Fortunately  this  can  be  experimentally  induced  by 
application  of  brief  stimuli  to  nerves  and  central 
tracts  leading  to  the  brain,  or  by  local  stimulation  of 
the  cerebral  structure  themselves.  On  the  other  hand, 
spontaneous  synchronizations  often  occur,  which  are 
imperfect  and  of  limited  extent  in  normal  conditions 
but  exaggerated  and  widespread  in  convulsive  states. 
In  any  case,  synchrony  is  essentially  a  feature  of  the 
slow  components  of  neuronal  activity.  Spikes  usually 
appear  in  complete  asynchrony  and  remain  prac- 
tically undetectable  with  macroelectrodes,  whereas 
the  microelectrode  technique  is  well  fitted  for  spike 
recording.  Thus  these  two  approaches  are  more  or 
less  compleinentary. 

In  addition  to  the  basic  factors  which  determine 
the  course  of  elementary  electrogenic  processes  at  the 
neuronal  level,  many  others  come  into  play  and  com- 
bine in  various  ways  to  generate  the  different  forms 
of  complex  brain  potentials,  transitory  evoked  po- 
tentials, periodic  waves,  or  steady  gradients.  All  the 
characteristics  of  a  multiplicity  of  elements — number, 
density,  internal  organization  and  extrinsic  relations 
— take  part  in  the  final  result  but  cannot  always  safely 
be  inferred  froin  it. 

Chance  distribution  of  elementary  properties,  such 
as  latencies,  excitability  levels,  is  the  familiar  statistical 
aspect  first  to  be  considered  here.  Then  corne  the 
problems  related  to  the  physical  conditions  of  recep- 
tion: recording  may  be  superficial  or  deep;  electrodes 
are  of  various  types,  numbers  and  placements;  dis- 
tribution of  potentials  in  a  volume  conductor  of 
limited  extent  has  its  intangible  laws  which  can  be 
applied  here  only  with  very  crude  approximation. 

A  further  step  considers  the  role  of  architectonic 
organization,  a  factor  of  primary  importance  here, 
for  physical  as  well  as  for  physiological  reasons. 
Laminar,  nuclear  or  reticular  structures  cannot 
produce  similar  electric  fields,  and  the  field  configura- 
tion in  each  particular  case  depends  upon  the  way  in 
which  neurons  of  different  kinds  are  distributed  and 
oriented  within  the  structure.  For  instance,  surface 
potentials  derived  from  the  cerebral  cortex  can  be 
thought  of  as  engendered  by  polarized  leaflets,  the 
unit  components  of  which  are  parallel  dipoles  formed 
by  the  long  pyramidal  neurons.  Synchrony  itself  is 
favored  by  such  regularity  of  internal  organization,  as 
a  result  of  a  certain  congruence  between  the  spatial 
order  existing  in  the  neural  structures  and  that  dis- 
played by  the  total  electric  field  produced  by  the 
active  elements  of  these  structures.  However,  this 
assumption  of  a  field  effect,  although  very  likely  in 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


closely  packed  and  orderly  arranged  populations  of 
neurons,  has  not  been  universally  accepted.  Inter- 
actions can  of  course  also  occur  through  synaptic 
connections  at  short  distance  and  these  must  be  taken 
into  account.  Most  frequently,  synchrony  in  a  popu- 
lation of  neurons  can  be  explained  by  the  triggering 
action  of  a  common  pacemaker  to  which  these  neurons 
are  linked;  it  thus  depends  on  the  gross  connections 
within  the  brain  or  '  tractology'.  But  the  problem 
remains  of  how  the  units  in  a  pacemaker  are  them- 
selves synchronized. 

This  leads  us  to  a  major  aspect  of  brain  potentials, 
their  frequent  appearance  in  regularly  rhythmic  se- 
quences. The  origin  of  these  periodic  activities  has 
been  the  object  of  discussions  and  controversies.  A 
pluralistic  attitude  seems  to  be  the  wisest,  the  rhythmic 
state  being  only  a  formal  appearance  that  may  be 
produced  by  diverse  causes. 

It  may  first  be  nothing  else  than  the  amplified  ex- 
pression of  an  elementary  autorhythmic  property  of 
some  neurons  whereby  they  emit  pulses  or  become 
the  site  of  local  oscillatory  states.  This  is  so  commonly 
encountered  in  microphysiological  experiments  with 
isolated  elements  that  one  can  hardly  douljt  that  this 
mechanism  operates  at  times  in  the  central  nervous 
system.  But  neurons  in  the  brain  never  work  in  isola- 
tion and  the  factors,  synaptic,  electrotonic  or  ephaptic, 
intervening  in  synchronization  must  aflPect  the  proper- 
ties of  the  autorhythmic  generators.  It  may  even  be 
that  rhythmicity  owes  its  existence,  in  many  cases,  to 
some  particular  arrangement  of  the  neuronal  connec- 
tions in  the  grey  matter.  Several  mechanisms  have 
been  proposed  which  are  possible  but  not  definitively 
demonstrated.  For  instance,  alternating  states  of 
excitation  and  inhibition,  with  their  corresponding 
opposite  electric  signs,  may  appear  by  virtue  of 
reciprocal  connections  between  the  generating 
neurons,  as  proposed  by  Jung  (33);  or  closed  chains 
of  neurons,  which  have  been  traced  through  central 
structures  by  Lorente  de  No  (41),  may  open  the  way 
to  recurring  pulses  activating  a  homogeneous  pool  of 
neurons. 

It  seems  more  sound  to  many  neurophysiologists  to 
replace  these  postulated  effects  of  a  rigid  circuitry  by 
others  attributable  to  the  properties  of  diffuse  net- 
works.  Neural   nets  finelv  wo\en   with   short,   inter- 


connected neurons  are  present  almost  everywhere  in 
the  grey  matter.  A  certain  average  level  of  intrinsic 
activity  may  be  maintained  within  these  structures 
by  an  incessant  and  random  circular  reactivation  of 
their  elements.  This  results  in  an  asynchronous 
bombardment  of  the  neurons  responsible  for  re- 
cordable potentials.  The  determining  factor  of 
periodicity  is  then  the  recovery  cycle  of  these  neurons. 
This  mechanism,  first  postulated  by  Eccles  (22,  23), 
has  received  strong  support  from  the  experiments 
of  Burns  on  isolated  slabs  of  cortex  (11,  12). 

Finally,  steady  potential  gradients  within  large 
assemblies  of  neurons  and  their  slow  modifications 
under  certain  conditions  appear  to  be  correlated  with 
spontaneous  or  e\oked  activities  in  the  grey  matter, 
according  to  the  views  of  O'Leary  and  his  collabora- 
tors (28;  see  Chaper  XIII).  The  correlations  may 
express  cause-effect  relationships  in  either  direction. 
For  instance,  long-lasting  after-bursts  in  isolated 
slabs  of  cerebral  cortex  have  been  related  by  Burns 
(13,  14)  to  gradients  that  appear  as  the  consequence 
of  different  recovery  rates  of  the  resting  membrane 
potentials  at  the  two  ends  of  particular  neurons.  One 
more  factor  capable  of  inducing  rhythmic  states  has 
thus  been  revealed.  But,  since  the  pioneer  studies  of 
Dusser  de  Barenne  et  al.  (20,  21),  Libet  &  Gerard 
(39)'  Jasper  &  Erickson  (32),  Leao  (37)  and  the 
recent  investigators  just  mentioned,  very  few  workers 
have  been  tempted  by  the  delicate  techniques  in- 
volved in  direct  current  recordings.  These  may  how- 
ever represent  the  next  fruitful  ad\ance  of  brain 
electrophysiology. 

Brain  potentials  and  their  rhythms  are  the  net 
result  of  a  conjunction  of  many  heterogeneous  factors 
— physical  conditions,  anatomical  organization,  statis- 
tical effects  and  the  differential  properties  of  the 
neuron  segments — implicated  in  different  ways. 
Consequently,  brain  potentials  are  able  only  to  reveal 
a  limited  aspect  of  cerebral  activity  and  must  always 
be  suspected  of  giving  a  distorted  picture  of  the  real 
events.  This  is  why  it  is  so  important  to  arrive  at  a 
better  understanding  of  their  elaboration,  for,  cor- 
rectly interpreted,  they  remain  the  unrivalled  signs 
of  what  occurs  in  the  intimacy  of  cerebral  tissue  and 
the  main  basis  for  explaining  brain  functions. 


REFERENCES 

I.   .\drian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.  88:  uy,   1936. 

■2.   .Adrian,  E.  D.   and  B.  H.   C.   Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  30: 

I.  1933- 
3.  Albe-Fessard,  D.  and  P.  Buser.  J.  phsiol.,  Paris  45:   14, 

1953- 


4.  Albe-Fessard,  D.  and  P.  Buser.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  47:  67, 

'955- 

5.  Amassi.an,   V.    E.   Eleclroencephalog.  &   Clin,  .\europhysiol.   5: 

4  "5.  1953- 

6.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  Psychol,  .\lonogr.  94:  30,  1933. 


BRAIN    POTENTIALS    AND    RHYTHMS INTRODUCTION 


259 


7.  Beck,  A.  and  N.  Cvbulski.  ^entralbl.  Physiol.  6:1,  1892. 

8.  Berger,  H.  Arch.  Psychiat.  87:  527,  1929. 

9.  Bishop,  G.  H.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  103:  213,  1933. 

10.  Bremer,  F.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  118:  1241,  1935. 

11.  Burns,  B.  D.  J    Physiol.    11 1:  50,   1950. 

12.  Burns,  B.  D.  J.  Physiol.  112:  156,  1951. 

13.  Burns,  B.  D.  J.  Physiol.  125:  427,  1954. 

14.  Burns,  B.  D.  J.  Physiol.  127:  168,  1955. 

15.  BusER,  P.  J.  physioL,  Paris  48:  49,  1956. 

16.  Caton,  R.  43d  Annual  Meeting  Bril.  Med.  Assoc.  Edinburgh, 
Aug.   3-6,    1875. 

17.  Chang,  H.  T.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Ojianl.  Biol.  17: 
189,  1952. 

18.  Clare,  M.  H.  and  G.  H.  Bishop.  FJectmencephalng.  &  Clin. 
.Neurophysiol.   7:  85,    1955. 

19.  Danilewski,  V.J.  Physiol.  Sbornike  2:  77,  1891. 

20.  DUSSER   DE  BaRENNE,  J.    G.,   \V.    S.    McCuLLOCH   AND   L.    F. 

NiMS.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  10:  277,  1937. 

21.  DussER  DE  Barenne,  J.  G.,  C.  S.  Marshall,  W.  S. 
MgCulloch  and  L.  F.  Nims.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  124:  631, 
■938- 

22.  Eccles,    J.    C.    Electroencephalog.    Z^    Clin.    Neurophysiol.    3: 

449.  '95I- 

23.  Eccles,  J.  C.  The  .Xeurophysiological  Basis  of  Mind.  Oxford: 
Clarendon  Press,   1953. 

24.  Fischer,  M.  H.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  120:  161,  1932. 

25.  Fleischl  von  Marxow,  E.  ^entralhl.  Physiol.  4:  537,  1890. 

26.  Gerard,  R.  W.  and  B.  Lieet,  Am.  J.  Psychiat.  96:  1 127, 
1940. 

27.  Gerard,  R.  VV.,  W.  H.  Marshall  and  L.  J.  Saul.  Am. 
J.Physiol.  109:38,  1934. 

28.  Goldring,  S.  and  J.  L.  O'Learv.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  6:  189,  1954. 

29.  Gozzano,  M.   Riv.  neural.  8:  212,    1935. 


30.  Grundfest,  H.  and  D.  P.  Purpura.  Nature,  London  8: 
416,  1956. 

31.  Jasper,  H.  H.  Psychol.  Bull.  34:  411,  1937. 

32.  J.-KSPER,   H.   H.   and  T.   C.    Erickson.  J.  Neurophysiol.   4: 

333-    '94' • 

33.  Jung,  R.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  4:  57,  1954. 

34.  Jung,  R.,  R.  von  Baumgarten  and  G.  Baumgartner. 
Arch.  Psychiat.   189:  521,   1952. 

35.  Kornmuller,  a.  E.  Forischr.  Neurol.,  Psychiat.  5-  419,  1933. 

36.  Kornmuller,  A.  E.  Die  Bioelektrischen  Erscheinungen  der 
Hir nrindenj elder .   Leipzig:  Thieme,    1937. 

37.  Lead,  A.   P.  J.  .Neurophysiol.    10:  409,    1947. 

38.  Li,  C.  L.  and  H.  H.  Jasper.  J.  Physiol.  121:  117,  1953. 

39.  LiBET,  B.  and  R.  W.  Gerard.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  438,  1941. 

40.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,   R.  J.  Neurophysiol.    i  :  207,    1938. 

41.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  29:  207,  1947. 

42.  MoRuzzi,  G.,  J.  M.  Brookhart  and  R.  S.  Snider.  Fed. 
Proc.  8:   113,    1949. 

43.  Phillips,  C.  G.  Quarl.  J.  E.xpcr.  Physiol.  41  :  58,   1956. 

44.  Prawdicz-Neminskv,    W.    \V.   ^entralbl.    Physiol.    27:   951, 

19I3- 

45.  Prawdicz-Neminskv,  W.    W.   .irch.  ges.  Physiol.  209-  363, 

'923- 

46.  Renshaw,  B.,  a.  Forbes  and  B.  R.  Morison.  J.  .Xeuro- 
physiol.  3 :  74,  1 940. 

47.  RoiTBAK,  A.  \.  Bioelectric  Phenomena  oj  the  Cerebral  Cortex 
(in  Russian).  Tiflis:  Publ.  House,  Acad,  of  Sciences, 
Georgian  S.  S.  R.,   1955. 

48.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hasp.  94:  238,  1954. 

49.  Tasaki,  I.,  E.  H.  PoLLEV  AND  F.  Orrego.  J.  Neurophysiol. 

17:  454.  1954- 

50.  Wang,  G.  H.  Chinese  J.  Physiol.  8:  121,  1934. 


CHAPTER    X 


Identification  and  analysis  of  single  unit  activity 
in  the  central  nervous  system 


KARL     FRANK     I     N'llional  Institute  of  Neurological  Diseases  and  Blindness, 
j     National  Institutes  oj  Health,  Bethesda,  Maryland 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Single  Unit  Techniques 
Single  Fiber  Isolation 
Microelectrodes 
Metal  electrodes 
Micropipettes 
Construction 
Filling 

Electrical  properties  of  micropipettes 
Resistance 
Capacitance 
Tip  potential 
Frequency  response 
Amplifiers 
Identification  of  Single  Units 
Position 
Axons 

Damage  to  Penetrated  Units 
Primary  Sensory  Fibers 
Motoneurons 
Interneurons 
Slow  Potentials 

Steps  in  the  Development  of  Cell  Spikes 
Stimulation  Through  Microelectrodes 


SOMEWHERE  IN  THE  MIDDLE  of  the  widc  range  of  ap- 
proaches to  neurophysiology  is  the  study  of  the 
physiological  properties  of  the  individual  neurons  in 
the  central  nervous  system.  In  order  to  put  this  ap- 
proach in  its  proper  perspective,  it  should  be  empha- 
sized that  the  functioning  of  the  central  nervous 
system  as  a  whole  is  as  difficult  to  predict  from  the 
known  properties  of  each  of  its  cellular  components  as 
are  the  properties  of  the  units  from  the  behavior  of 


the  whole.  While  it  is  not  sufficient  for  understanding 
the  nervous  system,  it  is  necessary  to  study  the  indi- 
vidual nerve  cells,  their  various  structures,  their  differ- 
ent patterns  of  activity  and  the  mechanisms  operat- 
ing to  yield  and  to  limit  such  activity. 

A  great  body  of  knowledge  has  been  built  up  about 
the  nature  of  neurons  in  the  peripheral  nervous 
system.  One  of  the  most  fruitful  approaches  to  the 
study  of  single  units  in  the  central  nervous  system  is 
through  the  extension  and  elaboration  of  these 
peripheral  findings.  Properties  of  peripheral  ner\-e 
fibers,  of  sensory  and  motor  end  organs  and  of 
ganglion  cells  are  continually  being  dfscovered  within 
the  spinal  cord  and  brain,  and  these  properties  must 
be  carefully  checked  lest  differences  or  totally  new 
mechanisms  be  thereby  overlooked. 

Neurons  show  a  variety  of  changes  which  can  be 
observed  in  a  study  of  their  activity.  Optical,  thermal 
and  mechanical  changes  have  all  been  ob.served  to 
accompany  activity  in  nerve,  but  chemical  and 
especially  electrical  changes  have  been  used  most 
extensively  to  acquire  our  present  knowledge  of 
single  cell  neurophysiology. 


SINGLE    UNIT    TECHNIQUES 

Techniques  for  single  unit  studies  all  require  some 
means  of  isolating  the  unit  to  be  studied.  Cells  which 
cannot  be  isolated  anatomically  due  to  their  many 
connections  with  other  cells  may  sometimes  be  iso- 
lated electrically.  This  may  be  done  either  by  limit- 
ing nervous  activity  at  a  particular  time  to  the  unit 


261 


262 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


under  study  or  by  restricting  the  sensitivity  of  the 
recording  device  to  the  electrical  activity  of  that  unit 
alone. 

Single  Fiber  Isolatian 

Bv  carefully  dissecting  trunks  of  peripheral  nerve 
fibers  or  spinal  roots  into  smaller  and  smaller  bun- 
dles, it  is  often  possible  to  reach  a  size  of  filament 
within  which  only  a  few  or  even  only  one  nerve  fiber 
remains  functional  (see  Chapter  III).  Such  a  fiber 
can  then  be  stimulated  or  its  action  potential  recorded 
in  isolation.  This  technique  has  been  used  to  study 
the  patterns  of  activity  of  indi\"idual  motoneurons  in 
response  to  various  types  of  excitation  and  inhibition 
(i,  6,  21,  31,  37,  38,  44).  EssentialK'  the  same  tech- 
nique has  been  used  by  Fessard  &  Matthews  (24),  by 
Kato  et  al.  (40)  and  by  others  to  limit  afferent  im- 
pulses to  those  carried  by  a  single  fiber.  Such  single 
unit  aff^erent  impulses  have  been  shown  to  produce 
long-lasting  potential  changes  on  nearby  afferent 
fibers  (the  dorsal  root  potential)  and  reflex  excitation, 
provided  other  excitatory  pathways  had  pre\iously 
brought  the  reflex  nearly  to  threshold.  The  same  tech- 
nique has  enabled  Adrian  &  Zotterman  (2)  and 
Cohen  et  al.  (13)  to  study  activity  patterns  of  single 
sensory  receptors.  Finally,  Fatt  (22)  has  similarly  iso- 
lated a  single  motor  fiber  from  its  peripheral  nerve.  A 
nerve  impulse  was  conducted  antidromically  by  this 
fiber,  and  its  invasion  of  the  motoneuron  cell  body 
and  dendrites  was  followed  by  mapping  the  electrical 
potential  field  produced  in  the  surrounding  volume 
conductor  of  the  spinal  cord. 

MiCROELECTRODES.  In  the  experiment  just  described, 
isolation  of  the  unit  was  achie\-ed  b\'  the  i^eripheral 
nerve  dissection  which  permitted  only  one  fiber  to  be 
stimulated.  An  alternative  method  of  isolating 
responses  from  single  units  is  to  use  a  microelectrode 
which  is  small  enough  to  be  selectively  sensitive  to  the 
activity  of  a  single  cell.  With  such  a  method  it  is  not 
necessary  to  interfere  with  the  patterns  of  activity  of 
other  cells  in  the  nervous  system  since  the  potentials 
they  generate  in  the  microelectrode  are  small  com- 
pared with  the  signals  being  studied.  This  method  of 
isolation  requires  that  the  microelectrode  be  of  small 
enough  dimensions  to  permit  it  to  be  placed  closer  to 
the  unit  to  be  studied  than  to  other  acti\e  units.  A 
wire  or  needle  sharpened  to  a  diameter  of  about  o.oi 
mm  (10  m)  and  insulated  except  at  its  tip  satisfies  this 
requirement  for  many  nerve  cells  in  the  spinal  cord 
and  brain  (7).  Much  larger  electrodes  (50  to  too  ^) 
appear  to  damage  individual  units  (50)  while  still  far 


enough  away  from  them  so  that  their  potential  fields 
are  masked  by  the  background  activity  of  other  cells. 
Smaller  metal  recording  electrodes  require  special 
techniques.  A  number  of  these  ha\e  Ijeen  de\Tloped 
and  are  described  below. 

MET.AL  ELECTRODES.  .Svactichin  (52)  dcscribes  a  tech- 
nique for  filling  fine  glass  pipettes  with  silver  solder 
(fig.  i.-l),  thus  providing  a  very  small  metallic  record- 
ing surface  down  to  less  than  i  /i,  well  insulated  by  a 
smooth  tapering  glass  shaft.  The  tips  of  these  electrodes 
are  plated  with  rhodium  and  then  coated  with 
platinum  black  to  prevent  oxidation  and  to  increase 
the  surface  area. 

Howlancl  et  al.  (35)  has  also  used  a  glass  insulated 
metal  wire  prepared  by  drawing  a  glass  pipette  down 
onto  a  I  2  ;u  nichrome  wire  which  had  previously  been 
passed  through  the  tube.  While  single  unit  acti\ity 
has  been  recorded  with  these  electrodes  in  the  cat's 
spinal  cord,  they  are  not  very  satisfactory  for  this 
piu"pose  and  ha\e  been  u.sed  mostly  to  record  the 
composite  responses  of  fiber  tracts  and  cell  groups. 

Dowben  &  Rose  (16^  have  devised  a  inore  satis- 
factory metal  microelectrode  which  they  have  used 
with  consideraljle  success  in  studving  unitary  activity 
of  the  thalamus.  These  workers  have  made  use  of  the 
low  melting  point  of  the  metal  indium  which  permits 
them  to  fill  predrawn  glass  pipettes  of  3  to  5  ^i  tip 
diameter  with  the  metal  (fig.  iB).  The  metal  surface 
at  the  tip  is  coated  with  gold  and  then  platinum 
black  which  probably  reduces  the  electrical  resistance 
of  the  metal-to-electroK  te  surface  due  to  the  porous 
nature  of  the  platinum  black  and  reduces  the  rate 
at  which  the  surface  becomes  polarized  during  the 
passage  of  electrical  currents. 

Perhaps  the  ultimate  in  fine  tipped  metal  micro- 
electrodes  is  produced  by  the  electroetching  and 
polishing  technique  (32).  Hubel  (36),  applying  this 
technique  to  a  tungsten  wire  which  he  then  insulates 
with  a  clear  lacquer  down  to  the  tip,  has  produced  an 
electrode  of  0.4  /j  tip  diameter  with  which  he  has  re- 
corded the  intracellular  potentials  of  motor  horn 
cells  in  the  spinal  cord  of  the  cat  (fig.  iC). 

A  metal-electrolyte  interface  or  junction  behaves 
somewhat  like  a  condenser  due  to  polarization 
eflPects.  In  general  the  greater  the  current  density  at 
the  junction  the  more  rapidly  it  becomes  polarized. 
Thus,  as  the  tip  of  a  metal  electrode  becomes  smaller 
the  difficulty  with  polarization  increases.  Successful  at- 
tempts have  been  made  to  reduce  polarization  by 
coating  the  microelectrode  tip  with  platinum  black 
(16,  52)  and  by  using  amplifiers  which  draw  very 
small  currents  (Bak,  A.   F.,  manuscript  in  prepara- 


IDENTIFICATION    AND   ANALYSIS   OF  SINGLE   UNIT  ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


263 


tion;  and  36).  Electrode  polarization  causes  a  varia- 
tion in  sensitivity  of  the  electrode-recording  device 
combination  with  the  frequency  of  the  recorded  po- 
tential. Lower  frequencies  are  more  attenuated  than 
higher  frequencies,  and  slow  changes  in  potential  tend 
to  be  lost.  In  addition  there  is  sometimes  a  fluctuation 
in  contact  potential  between  metals  and  electrolytes. 
Because  of  these  shortcomings,  metal  microelectrodes 
have  been  used  more  for  recording  extracellular 
transient  potential  changes  and  patterns  of  unitary 


activity  than  for  studying  the  exact  form  of  the  po- 
tential waves  or  lasting  potential  changes. 

.viiCROPiPETTES.  The  problems  inherent  in  recording 
from  a  metal  surface  of  very  small  area  are  in  part 
avoided  by  using  a  glass  pipette  filled  with  an  electro- 
lyte. With  such  an  electrode  the  electrolyte-metal 
boundary  is  moved  back  from  the  tip  to  the  shank 
where  a  long  wire  provides  a  large  surface  area  (fig. 
2.4).  This  electrode,  while  not  suffering  from  a  limited 


■••III 


I 


FIG.  I. A.  Silver-filled  glass  micropipettcs  a  and  c  show  glass-insulated  microelectrodes  with  tips 
containing  no  metal,  h  and  d  are  the  same  electrodes  after  electrolytic  filling  with  rhodium.  Scale: 
10  M  [From  Svaetichin  (52).]  B.  Stages  in  preparation  of  an  indium-filled  micropipctte:  a,  capillary 
tubing  half  filled  with  low  melting  point  metal;  b,  the  capillary  after  tip  is  drawn  but  before  metal  is 
pushed  to  fill  it  completely;  c,  electrode  tip  showing  platinum  black  deposit.  Calibrations:  i  mm  for 
a  and  A ;  10  /u  for  c.  [From  Dowben  &  Rose  ( 1 6).]  C  Lacquered  timgsten  microelectrodes  sharpened  by 
electropolishing:  a,  electronmicrograph  of  uncoated  wire;  b,  optical  photograph  of  three  coated 
electrodes  immersed  in  water  to  show  normal  variation  in  coating.  [From  Hubel  C36).] 


Glass  with  Pf  Plating 


Glass  1        Stainless  Steel 


40  35  30  25  20 


MILLIMETERS 


^M 


FIG.  2.-4.  (/(■//)  KCl  filled  microelectrode  used  for  intracellular  recording  showing  one  method  of 
mounting.  Platinum  lead  from  amplifier  contacts  inner  mercury  droplet  as  outer  shield  is 
clamped.  If  shield  is  driven  electrically  by  negative  capacity  amplifier,  platinum  coating  is  usually 
omitted.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (26).]  B.  (right)  Electronmicrograph  of  the  tip  of  a  similar  pipette. 
[From  Nastuk  &  Hodgkin  (47).] 


264 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


metal-electrolyte  surface  area,  has  plent)'  of  problems 
of  its  own.  But  in  addition  to  the  important  role  it 
has  played  in  peripheral  nerve  (see  Chapter  III), 
muscle  and  ganglion  studies,  the  micropipette  has 
provided  most  of  our  present  detailed  knowledge  of 
the  physiology  of  single  nerve  cells  in  the  central 
nervous  system.  This  is  partly  because  these  electrodes 
have  been  made  small  enough  to  penetrate  single 
nerve  cells  without  destroying  them,  thus  permitting 
measurement  of  the  potential  across  the  cell  mem- 
brane (figs.  iB  and  65).  For  this  reason  and  because 
the  use  of  this  type  of  electrode  appears  to  hold  much 
promise  for  future  investigations,  more  space  will 
be  devoted  to  its  description. 

Ling  &  Gerard  (42)  are  commonly  given  credit  for 
introducing  micropipettes,  probably  because  they 
were  the  first  to  record  action  potentials  from  inside 
single  muscle  fibers  with  these  electrodes.  However, 
Graham  &  Gerard  (30)  had  previously  recorded  the 
resting  membrane  potential  of  frog  muscle  fibers  with 
similar  fine  electrodes  filled  with  saline,  and  many 
workers  before  them  have  used  coarse  pipettes  for  re- 
cording electrical  potentials  as  in  the  use  of  small 
calomel  half-cells  and  agar  bridges  [references  to  this 
work  are  listed  by  Svaetichin  (52)]. 

CONSTRUCTION.  Ling  &  Gerard  (42)  made  micro- 
pipettes by  hand,  drawing  i  to  2  mm  glass  tubing 
down  to  a  fine  tip  in  an  oxygen-gas  microflame. 
Alexander  &  Nastuk  (4),  Livingston  &  Duggar  (43) 
and  others  have  devised  machines  for  pulling  pipettes 
similar  to  that  shown  in  figure  3.  This  machine  heats 
the  glass  tube  over  a  short  length  until  it  is  soft  enough 
to  be  drawn  out  by  the  pull  of  the  electromagnet. 
Increasing  the  heater  temperature  or  the  length  of  the 
heater  coil  produces  a  longer,  gentler  tapered  section 
on  the  pipette.  Figures  sA  and  B  show  the  size  and 
one  way  of  mounting  glass  micropipettes.  Another 
method  of  mounting  supports  the  tip  of  a  glass  micro- 
pipette on  a  I  mil  tungsten  wire  (59).  This  method 
has  been  used  to  permit  recording  of  intracellular  po- 
tentials from  muscle  where  movement  would  dis- 
lodge a  rigidly  mounted  pipette. 

FILLING.  The  size  of  the  tips  of  such  micropipettes 
ranges  upward  from  a  few  tenths  of  a  micron  (fig. 
2B).  The  problem  of  filling  them  with  an  electrolyte 
becomes  more  difficult  as  the  size  of  the  tip  is  reduced. 
Above  about  5  m  the  pipettes  can  be  filled  with  a 
syringe  by  expelling  the  air  from  the  tip.  Boiling  for 
several  hours  submerged  in  the  electrolyte  is  satis- 
factory for  all  but  the  smallest  tips  although  it  appears 


FIG.  3.  Vertical  micropipette  puller.  Upper  clamp  fixed; 
lower  clamp  pulled  down  by  solenoid,  gently  at  first  when 
glass  begins  to  .soften,  hard  just  before  pipettes  separate.  Tips 
are  drawn  down  out  of  heater  coil  which  is  turned  off  any  time 
after  pull  is  completed.  (Developed  at  National  Institutes  of 
Health,  Instrument  Section.) 


to  enlarge  them  somewhat.  N.  Tasaki  (55)  has  de- 
vised the  most  satisfactory  method  of  filling.  The 
pipettes  are  immersed  in  methyl  alcohol  in  a  chamber 
which  is  gradually  evacuated  until  the  alcohol  boils 
vigorously  for  a  few  minutes.  The  low  \iscosity  and 
low  boiling  point  of  the  alcohol  permit  the  finest  tips 
to  be  filled  quickly  and  without  damage.  The  alcohol 
can  then  he  replaced  by  the  desired  electrolyte  by 
dififusion  in  about  two  days.  Preboiling  and  filtering 
the  electrolyte  reduces  the  formation  of  air  bubbles 
and  clogging  by  foreign  particles.  The  micropipettes 
are  best  stored  in  alcohol  or  water  and  are  transferred 
to  the  desired  electrolyte  a  few  days  before  they  are 
needed. 

A  micropipette  can  be  filled  with  a  variety  of  differ- 
ent electrolytes.  Considerations  to  be  taken  into  ac- 
count are  electrical  conductivity,  similarity'  of  cation 
and  anion  mobilities,  the  possibility  of  damage  to  cells 
through  the  diffusion  of  the  electrolyte  out  of  the 
pipette  and  the  purpose  for  which  the  pipette  is  used. 


IDENTIFICATION    AND    ANALYSIS   OF   SINGLE    UNIT   ACTIVITY    IN    CENTRAL    NERVOUS   SYSTEM 


26s 


The  conductivities  of  several  electrolytes  which  have 
been  used  are  given  in  table  i.  The  rate  of  diffusion 
from  the  tips  varies  widely,  of  course,  but  the  magni- 
tude of  this  effect  can  be  seen  from  an  example  given 
by  Nastuk  &  Hodgkin  (47).  They  report  a  diffusion  of 
KCl  from  a  0.5  m  micropipette  filled  with  3  m  KCl  of 
6  X  to"'*  M  per  sec.  If  a  micropipette  maintaining 
this  flow  is  introduced  into  an  infinite  liquid  space, 
the  concentration  of  KCl  at  equilibrium  can  be  de- 
termined from  the  relation 


C\  = 


F 


4ir.vZ) 


+  c. 


where  C'^  is  the  uniform  concentration  ol  KCll  in  the 
space  before  introducing  the  pipette,  a  is  the  dis- 
tance from  the  pipette  tip,  F  is  the  rate  of  flow  of  KCl 
from  the  tip  and  D  is  the  difTusion  coefficient.  Apply- 
ing reasonable  values  for  the  electrode  described 
above  indicates  that  the  order  of  magnitude  of  the 
increase  in  concentration  of  KCl  at  a  distance  of  10 
H  from  the  tip  is  3  X  io~*  m  per  1.  This  may  be  com- 
pared to  the  figure  1.5  X  10"^  m  per  1.  taken  by 
Coombs  et  al.  (15)  as  the  concentration  of  K+  in  the 
intercellular  spaces  of  the  cat's  spinal  cord. 

When  a  micropipette  carries  an  electric  current, 
there  is  a  selective  migration  of  ions  through  the  tip 
superimposed  on  the  movement  by  diffusion  just  dis- 
cussed. If  the  ionic  concentration  in  the  pipette  is 
much  greater  than  that  outside  the  tip  then,  regardless 
of  its  direction,  the  current  will  be  carried  largely  by 
movement  of  ions  from  inside  to  outside  the  tip,  by 
anions  if  the  electrode  is  negative  and  by  cations  if  it 


TABLE  I .  Coiuhutivity  of  Solutions  Used  in  Microelectrodes* 


Solution 

23.S°C 

38°C 

KCl  (3  m) 

0.26 

0.27 

NaCl  (1%) 

0.018 

0.02 

NaCl  (2  m) 

0.14 

0.17 

K,S04  (0.6  m) 

0.08 

O.IO 

AgNOs  (sat.) 

0.20 

0.23 

CuClj  (sat.) 

0.08 

O.IO 

FeCb  (20%) 

0.075 

0.12 

Trypan  Red  (sat.) 

0.03 

0.03 

*  In  reciprocal  ohms  (mhos)  per  cm. 

Composition  of  solutions:  KCl  3  m  solution:  224  gm  KCl 
dissolved  and  diluted  to  i  liter.  NaCl  1*^0:  0.5  gm  NaCl  dis- 
solved and  diluted  to  50  cc.  NaCl  2  m  :  5.85  gm  NaCl  dissolved 
and  diluted  to  50  cc.  K.2SO4  0.6  m:  5.2  gm  KoSOj  dissolved 
and  diluted  to  50  cc.  AgNoj  saturated:  122  gra  AgNos  dis- 
solved in  100  cc  water.  CuClj  saturated:  no  gm  CuClj  dis- 
solved in  100  cc  water.  FeClj  20%:  20  gm  FeCh  dissolved  and 
diluted  to  100  cc.  Trypan  Red  saturated:  about  i  gm/ioo 
cc;  excess  filtered  off. 


is  positive.  When  the  mobilities  of  the  ion  species  are 
different,  the  electrical  conductivity  of  the  pipette 
will  change  with  the  direction  of  current  carried, 
and  the  electrode  will  show  rectification.  These 
properties  of  micropipettes  have  been  used  to  ad- 
vantage both  for  excitation  of  membranes  and  for  de- 
termining the  effects  of  specific  ions  on  the  behavior 
of  single  cells  (15). 

ELECTRic-^L  PROPERTIES  OF  MICROPIPETTES.  Resistance. 
The  electrical  resistance  of  a  micropipette  may  be 
thought  of  as  the  sum  of  the  resistance  of  a  truncated 
cone  of  the  inside  electrolyte  and  the  resistance  of  the 
voluine  conductor  around  the  tip.  If  the  tip  diameter 
is  less  than  i  n,  more  than  90  per  cent  of  the  resistance 
lies  in  the  last  10  /i  of  the  tip.  In  actual  practice  these 
electrodes  generally  range  from  a  few  to  several 
hundred  Mti. 

The  resistance  of  a  pipette  is  also  dependent  on  the 
direction,  amplitude  and  sometimes  on  the  dura- 
tion of  the  current  it  is  carrying.  For  very  small  cur- 
rents of  brief  duration,  the  electrode  usually  behaves 
either  like  a  pure  resistance  or  a  simple  rectifier.  The 
rectifier  action  of  pipettes  has  not  been  studied  sys- 
tematically for  a  large  number  of  electrolytes.  When 
even  small  currents  are  maintained  for  a  long  time, 
the  resistance  may  increase.  This  has  been  inter- 
preted by  Taylor  in  the  Appendix  to  Jenerick  & 
Gerard  C39)  as  a  movement  of  low-conductivity  ex- 
ternal electrolyte  into  the  tip  due  to  bound  surface 
charges  on  the  glass,  and  is  a  reversible  phenomenon. 
Larger  currents  delivered  in  pulses  at  constant  voltage 
may  cause  sudden  erratic  changes  in  resistance,  and 
the  amplitude  of  current  pulses  at  which  these  changes 
begin  generally  differs  with  polarity.  Sustained  ap- 
plication of  several  volts  across  a  micropipette  will 
often  increase  its  resistance  irreversibly  to  more  than 
10^0.  Some  form  of  clogging  in  the  extreme  tip  is  sug- 
gested, and  it  is  usually  possible  to  break  the  fine  tip 
by  gently  bumping  the  pipette  under  a  microscope, 
thus  reducing  its  resistance  to  a  usable  value.  A  pipette 
of  the  dimensions  described  above  might  have  a  re- 
sistance of  20  Mi2  for  a  current  of  up  to  about  io~' 
amp.  in  either  direction  and  pass  this  current  without 
markedly  departing  from  a  pure  resistance.  Tasaki 
(personal  communication)  has  been  able  to  select 
equally  fine,  hand-drawn  inicropipettes  carrying  up  to 
io~^  amp.  provided  the  external  volume  conductor 
was  sufficiently  acid. 

When  used  with  a  good  preamplifier  (see  page  267), 
the  ability  of  a  micropipette  to  carry  current  is  rela- 
tively unimportant  if  it  is  to  be  used  only  for  measur- 


266 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


ins;  potentials.  But  when  stimulating  or  polarizing 
currents  are  to  be  delivered  to  the  penetrated  cell  (see 
page  274)  or  when  ions  are  to  be  deli\ered  through 
the  tip  bv  iontophoresis  then  pipettes  must  be  selected 
for  their  current-carrying  ability. 

Cajiacilnnce.  The  ability  of  a  micropipettc  to  follow 
rapid  changes  in  the  electrical  potential  at  its  tip 
varies  inversely  with  the  electrical  capacitance  across 
its  walls  (see  below).  The  capacitance  across  the 
glass  wall  of  a  micropipettc  between  inside  and  out- 
side electrolytes  depends  upon  the  thickness  of  the 
wall  and  on  the  length  of  that  portion  of  the  pipette 
which  is  immersed  in  the  external  \olume  conductor. 
Since  in  a  drawn  glass  pipette  the  ratio  of  wall  thick- 
ness to  diameter  remains  approximately  constant  the 
capacity  is  proportional  to  the  immersed  length.  Frey- 
gang  (28)  calculates  the  capacity  across  a  pipette 
drawn  from  Pyrex  tubing  No.  7740  having  a  ratio 
OD/ID  of  2  as  0.4  /i/if  per  mm.  This  is  close  to  the 
measured  value  for  an  actual  pipette  of  0.37  fifif  per 
mm  of  immersion.  The  total  capacity  is  linear  with 
immersion  depth  except  for  a  minor  change  occurring 
as  the  shoulder  of  the  pipette  enters  the  external  con- 
ductor. Nastuk's  figure  of  i  /x^f  per  mm  would  indi- 
cate a  smaller  ratio  of  OD,  ID  (47). 

Tip  potential.  Nastuk  &  Hodgkin  (47),  who  used 
micropipettes  filled  with  3  m  KCl,  assumed  pro- 
visionally that  measurements  of  potential  made  with 
.such  pipettes  are  not  altered  by  a  junction  potential 
at  the  electrode  tip.  However,  Nastuk  (46),  del  Cas- 
tillo &  Katz  (17)  and  Adrian  (3)  showed  that  with 
many  such  fine  tipped  pipettes  there  is  a  potential 
diflference  across  the  tip  which  may  be  as  large  as  70 
mv,  inside  negative,  and  that  this  tip  potential  may 
change  suddenly  with  movement  of  the  pipette  in 
muscle  or  nervous  tissue.  Thus  measurements  of 
steady  potential  differences,  e.g.  the  membrane  po- 
tential of  a  cell,  will  be  in  error  if  the  tip  potential  of 
the  micropipettc  differs  at  two  points.  Such  a  differ- 
ence can  arise  in  two  ways.  The  tip  potential  of  the 
pipette  may  be  changed  by  clogging  or  unclogging  the 
tip  as  it  moves  through  the  nervous  tissue.  In  this  case 
alternate  measurements  at  two  points  would  not 
generally  be  expected  to  repeat  the  error.  A  more  con- 
sistent error  will  be  encountered  if  the  two  points 
whose  potential  difference  is  to  be  measured  are  in 
regions  of  different  ionic  composition,  and  if  the  tip 
potential  of  the  micropipettc  is  different  in  the  two 
regions.  Adrian  (3),  attempting  to  clarify  this  point, 
measured  the  tip  potential  of  a  series  of  pipettes  in 
both  100  mmole  KCl  and  100  mmole  NaCl.  He  found 
that  the  difference  in  tip  potential  in  the  two  solutions 


was  proportional  to  the  tip  potential  in  100  mmole 
KCl.  Adrian  considers  the  mechanism  of  the  tip  po- 
tential to  be  a  selective  reduction  in  mobility  of  some 
of  the  ions,  particularly  that  of  the  anion  and  probably 
due  to  some  form  of  blocking.  He  argues  that  if  a 
pipette  has  a  small  tip  potential  it  is  less  likely  to 
show  a  change  or  introduce  an  error  due  to  tip  po- 
tential. On  this  basis  he  measures  the  resting  potential 
of  muscle  membrane  by  selecting  only  those  pipettes 
whose  tip  potentials  are  less  than  5  mv,  inside  nega- 
ti\e.  \Vhile  it  is  true  that  such  a  pipette  would  be  ex- 
pected to  introduce  only  a  small  error  of  i  or  2  mv  in 
the  measurement  of  potential  difference  between  two 
test  solutions  (100  mmole  KC'.l  and  100  mmole  NaCl), 
there  seems  to  be  less  certainty  that  a  pipette  having 
an  initially  small  tip  potential  will  not  increase  its  tip 
potential  during  its  movement  through  tissue.  In  the 
particular  case  of  the  resting  potential  across  a  cell 
memijrane,  errors  introduced  by  variation  of  tip 
potential  cannot  be  eliminated  by  repeated  compari- 
sons since  the  cell  memijrane  is  generally  damaged  by 
repeated  penetration.  For  this  reason,  micropipettc 
measurements  of  steady  potential  difference  between 
different  points  in  the  nervous  system  are  subject  to 
considerable  uncertainty  at  the  present  time. 

Frequency  response.  Metal  microelectrodes  are  gen- 
erallv  poor  for  measurement  of  d.c.  or  of  very  slowly 
varying  potentials  as  described  above,  but  at  higher 
frequencies  their  impedance  drops  to  relatively  low 
values.  The  resistance  of  a  glass  micropipettc  on  the 
other  hand  is  quite  independent  of  frequency  at  low 
frequencies.  This  does  not  mean  that  an  ordinary 
amplifier  connected  to  a  micropipettc  necessarily  re- 
cords biological  potentials  with  a  good  frequency 
response.  Indeed,  this  is  one  of  the  failings  of  these 
electrodes. 

A  micropipettc  must  generally  pass  through  a 
region  of  grounded  volume  conductor  before  reaching 
the  highlv  localized  region  whose  potential  is  to  be 
recorded.  The  capacity  across  the  glass  wall  between 
the  inside  electrolyte  and  this  external  volume  con- 
ductor (.see  above)  tends  to  reduce  the  high-fre- 
quency response  of  the  electrode.  Since  most  of  the 
resistance  of  the  pipette  is  located  very  near  its  tip,  it 
can  be  well  represented  in  such  an  application  by 
the  equivalent  circuit  of  figure  4.  If  the  input  im- 
pedance of  the  amplifier  is  high  enough  to  be  neglected 
in  comparison  to  Re  and  Ce,  a  sudden  change  in 
\  oltage  E  will  be  recorded  as  an  exponential  rise  hav- 
ing a  time  constant  r  =  Re  Ce;  that  is,  the  recorded 
N'oltage  \'  will  rise  to  about  63  per  cent  of  E  in  the 
time  T.  For  anv  form  of  voltage  signal  E  fed  in,  the 


IDENTIFICATION    AND    ANALYSIS  OF  SINGLE   UNIT  ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


267 


::  „ 

k 

|Re        J 

1 

AMP 

E 
7 

FIG.  4.  Approximate  equivalent  circuit  for  an  intracellular 
micropipette.  E,  cell  potential  to  be  recorded;  Re,  resistance 
of  microelectrodc  tip  plus  preparation,  Ce,  capacity  between 
electrolyte  inside  micropipette  and  grounded  volume  conductor 
in  which  it  is  immersed;  V,  potential  recorded  by  amplifier. 


J.  W.,  &  K.  S.  Cole,  manuscript  in  preparation; 
Lettvin,  J.  Y.,  &  B.  Howland,  manuscript  in  prepara- 
tion; Bak,  A.  F.,  manuscript  in  preparation.  This  type 
of  preamplifier,  by  utilizing  positive  feedback  to  the 
input  grid,  in  effect  adds  a  controlled  negative 
capacitance  in  parallel  with  the  positive  input  capaci- 
tance. By  minimizing  the  sum  of  these  capacities,  a 
considerable  improvement  can  be  made  in  the  record- 
ing time  constant.  Used  with  micropipettes  like  those 
described  above,  these  circuits  can  reach  an  effective 
input  capacitance  of  0.5  /i^uf  or  less. 


recorded  voltage  will  be 


V  =  E  - 


dV 
dt"" 


The  error  in  recorded  voltage  is  thus  proportional  to 
the  electrode  resistance,  to  its  capacity  to  ground  and 
to  the  first  derivative  of  the  recorded  voltage.  When 
the  input  voltage  is  a  sine  wave,  the  frequency  at 
which  the  recorded  voltage  is  reduced  to  0.7  E  is 
given  by 


f  = 


27rRp.  Cp 


Amplifiers.  Amplifiers  used  for  recording  from  micro- 
pipettes must  have  special  features  to  minimize  the 
effects  of  the  inherent  shortcomings  of  the  electrodes 
described  above.  Ideally,  the  amplifier  must  have  an 
input  resistance  which  is  high  in  comparison  with  the 
electrode  resistance,  a  low  enough  input  grid  current 
so  that  its  effects  at  the  tip  of  the  electrode  can  be 
neglected  and  a  negligible  effectixe  capacity  between 
input  and  ground.  These  requirements  of  the  ampli- 
fier do  not  include  voltage  gain  which  can  be  accom- 
plished in  a  following  amplifier.  Thus  the  preamplifier, 
as  it  is  usually  called,  is  actually  an  impedance  trans- 
former intended  to  isolate  the  source  of  potential  being 
measured  from  the  loading  effects  of  the  conventional 
voltage  amplifier. 

A  number  of  practical  preamplifiers  have  Ijeen  de- 
signed to  meet  these  special  requirements  with  varying 
degrees  of  success.  The  simplest  is  the  cathode  fol- 
lower circuit  of  which  a  good  example  is  that  de- 
scribed by  Nastuk  &  Hodgkin  (47).  This  circuit  gave 
an  overall  recording  time  constant  of  70  /z  sec.  when 
tested  with  a  22  Mli  pipette,  showing  an  effective 
input  capacitance  of  3.2  /i^if.  An  improved  circuit 
called  a  negative  capacity  amplifier  has  been  used  in 
various  forms  by  several  authors:  Solms  el  al.  (51); 
Woodbury  (58);  Wagner  &  MacNichol  (57);  Moore, 


IDENTIFICATION    OF    SINGLE    UNITS 


Position 


One  of  the  more  diHicult  problems  in  the  use  of 
micropipettes  is  the  determination  of  their  positions. 
Identification  of  the  structure  or  structures  generating 
the  various  potentials  recorded  by  the  micropipette 
requires  some  knowledge  of  the  relative  positions  of 
the  pipette  and  the  structures  which  might  be  respon- 
sible for  the  potentials.  Knowledge  of  which  neuron 
the  pipette  is  in  or  near  is  of  less  interest  than  the  kind 
of  neuron  and  the  position  of  the  pipette  relative  to 
the  various  parts  of  such  a  neuron.  Information  of 
this  kind  has  been  obtained  by  direct  microscopic  ob- 
servation, by  marking  techniques  and  by  inferences 
drawn  mostly  from  the  nature  of  the  potentials 
recorded. 

Direct  observation  of  the  microelectrodc  position  is 
limited  to  those  structures  which  can  be  dissected 
free  of  opaque  or  translucent  surrounding  tissues.  This 
technique  has  been  used  in  recording  from  muscle 
fibers  [see  bibliograph\-  in  Jenerick  &  Gerard  (39)]; 
peripheral  nerve  fibers  (Chapter  III,  54);  inverte- 
brate heart  ganglion  cells  (12);  eel  electroplaques  (5); 
dorsal  root  ganglion  cells  (53);  photoreceptor  cells 
C.33);  and  stretch  receptor  cells  (41).  Even  in  struc- 
tures where  this  technique  is  possible,  there  are 
severe  limitations.  The  tips  of  micropipettes  are  often 
submicroscopic  or  close  to  the  limit  of  resolution 
with  visible  light,  thus  requiring  ideal  conditions  of 
lighting,  numerical  aperture,  color  contrast  and 
contrast  of  indices  of  refraction.  Pressure  from  the 
pipette  often  distorts  the  ti.ssue,  and  even  under  the 
best  conditions  it  may  be  difficult  for  example  to  de- 
termine optically  whether  the  pipette  is  inside  or 
outside  of  a  particular  cell  wall.  However,  a  large 
part  of  the  present  body  of  knowledge  of  the  electro- 
physiology  of  single  cells  has  been  acquired  by 
studies  u.sing  this  technique. 


268 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


A  new  technique  has  been  proposed  for  extending 
direct  \ision  to  structures  within  the  central  nervous 
system.  This  is  accomplished  by  the  use  of  a  long  thin 
solid  cone  of  glass  mounted  in  front  of  the  microscope 
objective  which  extends  to  the  focal  plane.  Incident 
illumination  is  supplied  through  the  objective,  and 
the  cone  is  moved  through  the  tissue  until  the  desired 
object  appears  in  the  small  field  of  view  bounded  by 
the  flat  end  of  the  cone.  Using  this  technique,  moto- 
neurons have  been  seen  in  unfixed  tissue,  and  their 
nuclei  and  dendrites  are  clearly  visible.  It  is  hoped 
that  this  technique  can  be  used  for  determining  micro- 
electrode  position. 

Marking  techniques  have  been  used  extensively  for 
locating  gro.ss  microelectrodes.  One  method  produces 
a  lesion  by  passing  radio-frecjucncy  current  through 
an  insulated  metal  electrode  bared  at  its  tip.  The 
lesion  is  subsequently  made  visiijle  by  staining  the 
fixed  and  sectioned  material.  Another  method  using 
steel  electrodes  plates  off  iron  by  passing  a  current 
through  the  electrode.  The  region  where  the  iron 
has  been  deposited  is  then  stained  blue  by  ferrocy- 
anide  and  prepared  by  frozen  section  technique,  Mar- 
shall's modification  of  Hess'  method  (45).  The  resolu- 
tion of  about  half  a  millimeter  made  possible  by  these 
two  techniques  is  not  yet  great  enough  to  permit 
identification  of  single  cells  or  parts  of  cells.  Success 
in  marking  a  single  cortical  pyramidal  cell  has  been 
claimed  recently  by  Rayport  (48)  who  passed  a  cur- 
rent through  a  micropipette  filled  with  a  3  n 
FeNH4S04  solution.  Iron  ions  moved  by  electrophore- 
sis into  the  penetrated  cell  were  later  stained  blue  !)>• 
the  ferrocyanide  reaction  of  Hess  (34).  This  is  the 
only  case  known  to  the  author  of  a  single  nerve  cell 
penetrated  blindly  and  sub.sequently  identified 
visually. 

The  identification  of  nervous  structures  by  infer- 
ences made  from  the  potentials  recorded  from  micro- 
electrodes  is  uncertain  and  .subject  to  revision  when- 
ever further  information  modifies  the  assumptions  on 
which  such  inferences  are  based.  However,  pending 
the  development  of  a  more  direct  method,  the  study 
of  single  unit  activity  in  the  central  nervous  system 
is  limited  to  such  inferences  as  can  be  made  based  on 
comparisons  of  potentials  recorded  from  microelec- 
trodes in  the  central  nervous  system  with  those  re- 
corded from  cells  under  direct  vision  or  otherwise 
identified. 

Axons 

The  potentials  to  be  expected  from  axons  in  the 
central  ner\ous  system  can  be  predicted  from  meas- 


FIG.  5.  Effect  of  \olume  conductor  on  potentials  recorded 
by  intracellular  electrodes.  The  microclectrode  is  inserted  in  a 
fiber  of  a  ventral  root.  In  A  and  B  the  root  is  surrounded  by 
paraffin  oil,  in  C  and  D  the  oil  is  replaced  by  Ringer's  fluid. 
A  and  C,  stimulus  just  subthreshold  for  penetrated  fiber;  B 
and  D,  maximal  stimulus.  Calibration :  50  mv.  Time :  i  msec. 
[From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (26).] 


urements  made  on  peripheral  nerve  (see  Chapter 
III).  If  a  nerve  surrounded  by  an  insulating  medium  is 
made  to  conduct  a  synchronous  volley  of  impulses,  a 
fairly  large  action  potential  can  be  recorded  mono- 
polarly  from  a  gross  electrode  in  contact  with  the 
ner\e.  As  seen  in  figure  5^4,  a  microclectrode  simi- 
larly situated  records  the  same  large  external  action 
potential  with  respect  to  a  distant  electrode  on  inac- 
tive tissue.  If  the  microclectrode  then  passes  through 
the  membrane  of  a  fiber  participating  in  the  volley, 
the  action  potential  it  records  will  be  the  algebraic 
sum  of  the  outside  potential  previously  recorded  and 
the  action  potential  produced  across  the  fiber  mem- 
brane (fig.  5/}).  The  effect  of  a  volume  conductor, 
such  as  the  spinal  cord  or  brain  surrounding  the  active 
fibers,  can  be  simulated  b>'  replacing  the  insulating 
medium  with  a  conductor  such  as  saline.  Figure  5C 
shows  that  the  external  recording  is  then  markedly  re- 
duced and  the  action  potential  developed  across  the 
penetrated  fiber  membrane  is  recorded  by  an  internal 
microelectrode  with  little  distortion  (fig.  jD).  When 
the  intpulses  in  the  fibers  are  not  synchronous,  the  al- 
ready small  external  potential  field  becomes  negligi- 
ble; but,  whatever  the  external  field  may  be,  it  will 
be  approximately  recorded  by  an  electrode  inside  or 
outside  a  resting  fiber.  Thus  it  can  be  anticipated  that 


IDENTIFICATION    AND   ANALYSIS  OF  SINGLE   UNIT   ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


269 


extrinsic  potentials  inside  a  volume  conductor  such  as 
the  spinal  cord  or  brain  are  small  and  do  not  ap- 
preciably distort  the  potentials  recorded  by  micro- 
electrodes  inserted  in  active  neural  elements  except  in 
the  presence  of  large  synchronous  volleys  in  a  limited 
volume  conductor. 

Fi?;ure  6.4  is  a  section  of  the  lumbar  region  of  a  cat's 
spinal  cord  showing  cell  bodies  of  neurons  as  black 
dots.  A  line  drawn  across  such  a  section  indicates  the 
structures  which  may  be  encountered  by  a  micro- 
electrode  as  it  is  advanced  through  the  tissue.  A  very 
few  motoneuron  somata  but  many  small  cells  and  in- 
numerable fibers  will  be  in  the  path  of  the  electrode. 
Apparently  penetration  of  fibers  occurs  only  when 
very  fine  micropipettes  are  used.  With  coarser  elec- 
trodes the  majority  of  the  elements  which  can  be  im- 
paled behave  as  if  they  were  cell  somata.  Figure  6B 
shows  the  relative  sizes  of  a  cat's  motoneuron  and  a 
typical  glass  micropipette. 

The  potentials  recorded  from  a  microelectrode  as 
it  is  moved  through  a  cat's  spinal  cord  are  indicated 


A 


v..    ^%^- 


.  \ 


B  >'^^  ■  Y 

4f^\ 

^•^ 

T 

A 

FIG.  6.^4.  Section  of  cat's  spinal  cord  at  L6.  Thionin  stain  to 
show  cell  bodies.  B.  Methylene-blue  stain  of  unfixed  slice  of 
spinal  cord  showing  KCl  filled  micropipette  penetrating  a 
motoneuron  near  the  surface  of  the  slice.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes 
(26).] 


in  figure  7.  The  upper  extreme  of  this  potential  is  re- 
peatedly recorded  and  is  taken  to  indicate  the  poten- 
tial in  the  extracellular  spaces  since  it  is  close  to  the 
potential  recorded  from  the  fluid  conductor  on  the 
cord  surface.  The  negative  deflections  are  presumed  to 
indicate  penetration  or  destruction  of  cellular  mem- 
branes, on  analogy  with  peripheral  findings.  While 
some  of  the  negative  potentials  recorded  must  be 
from  neural  elements  since  they  are  correlated  with 
spikes  like  action  potentials,  others  may  be  from 
nonneural  elements  such  as  glia  cells. 

If  the  electrode  is  allowed  to  remain  in  a  position 
where  it  records  a  steady  negative  potential,  spikes  or 
action  potentials  can  generally  be  seen  occurring 
either  spontaneously  or  in  response  to  stimulation 
(figs.  ']A  and  5).  The  amplitude  of  these  action  po- 


r^ 


4,t 


v_4 


FIG.  7.  Simultaneous  records  taken  during  penetration  of  a 
cat's  spinal  cord  with  a  KCl  filled  micropipette.  / :  Carotid 
blood  pressure.  2:  Movement  of  the  electrode.  The  limit  of 
deflection  of  the  instrument  was  reached  by  a  movement  of  200 
M-  After  this  the  pen  jumped  back  and  began  recording  further 
movement  in  the  same  way.  Upward  deflection  indicates  in- 
creased penetration.  3:  Signals  from  shutters  of  the  cameras 
used  for  making  records  of  inserts  A  and  B.  ./ :  Record  of  elec- 
trode potential  relative  to  reference  electrode  on  vertebral 
column.  Note  potential  fluctuations  when  electrode  is  moved 
and  steady  negativity  when  it  is,  presumably,  inside  the  mem- 
brane of  a  unit.  Insert  A  shows  responses  of  the  penetrated  unit 
to  stimulation  of  a  ventral  root,  as  photographed  by  a  single 
frame  camera.  Insert  B  shows  a  strip  of  record  taken  by  a 
moving  film  camera  at  the  time  indicated  by  the  two  arrows  in 
^.  Calibrations:  /,  50  to  150  mm  Hg;  2,  200  m;  inserts  A  and  B 
and  4,  50  mv.  Time;  60  sec.  for  /,  2,  3  and  4;  i  msec,  for  A. 
[From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (26).] 


270 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tentials  is  related  to  the  more  or  less  steady  resting 
potential  as  shown  in  figure  8.  The  majority  of  units 
penetrated  show  spikes  larger  than  the  corresponding 
resting  potentials.  Occasionally,  small  or  large  spikes 
are  recorded  with  negligible  resting  potential,  and 
many  of  these  have  a  diphasic  positive-negative 
shape  as  shown  in  9^.  Spikes  accompanying  large 
steady  resting  potentials  may  be  either  brief  like 
those  recorded  from  dorsal  or  ventral  root  fibers,  as  in 
figure  gfi,  or  of  longer  duration,  as  in  figure  gC. 


0             • 

/ 

mV 

- 

.     0     °.        .• 

/ 

100 

- 

0 

0       • 

•      •.° 

^ 

5 

■ 

0 
0 

•  0  *'*      iP        ^  "^ 

a 

0 

0 

••A        *   ^^oo  ' 

> 

0 "     0 

•'*      j/  '      ° 

50 

- 

0 

.00  .0  °- 

00    /^* 

. 

0   °      .  • 

/                • 

0 

/ 

'e 

Q/ 

. 

0          '/^ 

• 

- 

/          ° 

0 

,/ 

0 

/ 

1              1 

1          1          1          1          1 

1     1     1     i 

50 


100  mV 


FIG.  8.  Plot  of  spike  amplitude  against  steady  voltage  as 
measured  from  presumed  e.xtracellular  potential  level.  One 
hundred  and  sixty-seven  units  from  penetrations  of  cats'  spinal 
cords.  Open  circles  from  units  identified  as  primary  afferent 
fibers;  tilled  circles  from  other  units.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes 
(26).] 


Damage  to  Penetrated  Units 

A  unit  may  be  considered  to  have  been  seriously 
damaged  when  the  potentials  recorded  from  it  de- 
crease rapidly  and  are  small  and  drawn  out;  and 
when  the  pattern  of  activity  recorded  differs  from  that 
obtained  before  penetration.  Minor  damage  cannot 
be  recognized,  and  the  degree  of  abnormality  due  to 
insertion  of  the  microelectrode  can  only  be  postulated 
in  a  number  of  cases. 

The  assumption  that  small  spikes  are  recorded  from 
damaged  structures  seems  to  be  contradicted  by  the 
observation  that  elements  producing  small  spikes 
may  respond  with  normal  patterns  to  orthodromic 
stimulation.  However,  this  is  probably  due  to  the 
fact  that  damage  can  occur  at  the  place  of  recording 
(axon)  without  involving  the  structures  responsible 
for  the  generation  of  the  response  (.soma  and  den- 
drites). 

Primary  Sensory  Fibers 

When  the  microelectrode  penetrates  a  primary 
afferent  fiber  after  it  has  entered  the  central  nervous 
system,  the  unit  can  still  be  provisionally  identified  as 
sensory  by  several  features,  a)  The  action  potential 
should  have  about  the  same  shape,  size  and  duration 
as  those  recorded  with  microelectrodes  in  peripheral 
nerves  or  dorsal  roots.  Of  course,  fine  afferent  branches 
may  not  meet  this  requirement  and  other  axons  may 
not  be  excluded.  h~)  If  conduction  latency  is  less  than 
about  0.5  insec,  it  may  i)e  presumed  that  no  synapse 
is  tra\'ersed.  This  criterion  cannot  be  used  if  long  con- 
duction paths  are  involved.  Also  this  presumption  ex- 
cltides  the  possibility  of  very  fast  synapses,  such  as 


FIG.  9..-!.  Diphasic  potential  recorded  from  a  cat's  motoneuron  just  prior  to  penetration  as  indicated 
by  subsequent  sudden  development  of  negative  resting  potential.  Artifact  indicates  shock  to  dorsal 
root.  Calibration :  20  mv.  Time :  i  msec.  B.  Brief  spike  presumably  from  inside  an  axon  in  the  cat's 
spinal  cord.  Calibration:  20  rav.  Time  i  msec.  C.  .Action  potential  from  a  cat's  motoneuron  following 
first  dorsal  and  then  ventral  root  stimuli.  Calibration:  ^o  mv.  Time:  i  msec. 


IDENTIFICATION    AND    ANALYSIS  OF  SINGLE   UNIT   ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL  NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


might  Ije  required  for  the  inhibitory  interneurons 
proposed  by  Eccles  et  al.  (20).  c)  The  unit  in  question 
should  follow  trains  of  stimuli  up  to  a  rate  of  at  least 
500  impulses  per  sec.  This  figure  may  eliminate  some 
small  afferent  fibers  and  may  let  in  some  synapses  of 
very  high  safety  factor  but  probably  separates  the 
vast  majority  of  postsynaptic  from  presynaptic  ele- 
ments. (T)  If  the  unit  responds  to  stimulation  of  a 
dorsal  root  or  peripheral  nerve  which  is  cut  distal  to 
the  point  of  stimulation,  then  there  should  be  only  one 
response  for  each  stimulus.  This  last  criterion  elim- 
inates afferent  fibers  carrying  dorsal  root  reflex  re- 
spon.ses  (to,  25,  56),  but  these  have  never  been  ob- 
served at  shorter  latencies  than  2.5  msec,  and  cannot 
follow  at  high  frequencies  (26).  i)  Primary  afferent 
fibers  not  separated  from  their  sensory  receptor  cells 
can  often  be  made  to  fire  repetitively  by  natural 
stimuli  such  as  touch,  pinch  or  stretch.  These  trains 
of  impulses  are  characterized  by  their  extremely  regu- 
lar rhythms  under  sustained  excitation  and  can 
readilv  be  distinguished  from  most  postsynaptic  ele- 
ments in  this  regarded.  Again  this  type  of  criterion 
tends  to  prevent  the  discovery  of  regularly  firing  post- 
synaptic elements  should  these  exist.  While  none  of 
the  above  criteria  is  definitive  alone,  together  they 
form  a  rather  satisfactory  method  for  identifying  pri- 
mary afferents  penetrated  by  microelectrodes  in  the 
central  nervous  system.  Using  these  criteria,  primary 
afferents  have  been  identified  as  deep  as  4.5  mm  below 
the  dorsal  surface  of  the  cat's  spinal  cord  (26). 

Motoneurovs 

Responses  from  motoneurons  may  be  identified  by 
their  correlation  with  antidromic  stimulation  with 
the  adoption  of  only  very  reasonable  assumptions. 
Microelectrodes  in  ventral  root  fibers  of  the  spinal 
cord  give  responses  to  ventral  root  shocks  which  are 
similar  to  those  recorded  from  dorsal  root  fibers. 
When  the  electrode  is  in  the  spinal  cord,  similar  short- 
latency  spikes  are  recorded  following  ventral  root 
stimulation  (fig.  loi?).  These  are  presumabK-  the  axons 
of  motoneurons. 

However,  another  type  of  short-latency  response 
may  be  recorded  following  ventral  root  stimuli  as 
shown  in  figure  lo.-l.  Brock  et  al.  (ii),  Frank  & 
Fuortes  (26)  and  Woodbury  &  Patton  (60)  showed 
that  these  responses  are  of  longer  duration  than  axon 
spikes  (figs.  lo.-l  and  E),  are  followed  by  a  long-lasting 
hyperpolarization  (fig.  15)  and  when  elicited  within 
a  critical  interval  following  a  previous  spike,  break  at 
the  inflection  point  on  the  rising  pha.se  as  shown  in 


U 


N>m 


FIG.  10.  Antidromic  conduction  block  in  cat's  motoneurons. 
Electrodes  inserted  in  the  cord  may  pick  up  two  types  of  short 
latency  responses  to  pairs  of  ventral  root  shocks.  In  unit  of 
column  A  the  response  to  the  second  of  a  pair  of  shocks  suddenly 
drops  to  30  to  40  per  cent  when  the  shock  interval  is  reduced 
below  a  critical  stimulus  interval  (about  i  o  msec,  here,  but 
often  much  shorter  or  longer).  Conduction  block  must  occur 
near  microelectrode  since  blocked  impulse  is  visible  there.  Unit 
of  column  B  shows  instead  a  smooth  decrease  in  height  of 
second  response  as  stimulus  interval  is  decreased.  Calibration: 
50  mv.  Time  in  both  columns :  i  msec.  (Note  diflferent  sweep 
speeds  in  A  and  B.)  Only  units  like  that  in  B  are  found  in  ventral 
roots.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (26).] 


figure  lo.-l.  The  short  latency  of  these  responses  identi- 
fies them  as  from  motoneurons,  and  the  inflection  in 
the  rising  phase  has  been  interpreted  as  due  to  a  loss 
in  safety  factor  for  conduction  for  axon  hillock  to 
soma  (i  I,  26).  If  one  accepts  that  the  block  in  conduc- 
tion at  the  critical  stimulus  interval  occurs  at  the  axon 
hillock  then,  since  only  elements  with  long-duration 
spikes  show  evidence  of  conduction  block,  it  may  be 
concluded  that  long  spikes  originate  upstreain  froin 
the  axon  hillock,  i.e.  in  the  cell  bodies  or  dendrites, 
and  short  spikes  originate  in  axons. 

The  role  of  motoneuron  dendrites  in  the  generation 
of  potentials  following  antidroinic  stimulation  has 
not  yet  been  settled.  Fatt  (22)  has  recorded   poten- 


272 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


B 


^^ 


/*-^ 


FIG.  1 1.  Responses  obtained  in  cat's  spinal  cord  and  identiKed 
as  from  interneurons  by  criteria  discussed  in  text.  A.  Spike 
duration  about  i  msec,  after  prespilce  potential,  presumably  a 
soma  response.  B.  Spike  duration  about  0.3  msec,  with  no 
prespike  potential,  presumably  from  an  axon.  Brief  postspike 
hyperpolarization  is  a  frequent  but  not  a  constant  finding  and 
may  indicate  damage  done  to  a  fiber  by  the  microelectrode. 
Calibration:  50  mv.  Time:  1  msec.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes 
(26).] 


tials  which  he  interprets  as  indicating  antidromic 
conduction  along  the  dendrites  for  a  distance  in  excess 
of  I  mm.  But  the  diphasic  responses  of  figure  9^4  can 
be  interpreted  as  due  to  the  sum  of  conductive  and 
reactive  currents  outside  .some  inactive  membrane, 
according  to  Freygang  (28).  If  this  interpretation  is 
correct,  then  at  least  some  of  the  dendrites  of  moto- 
neurons probably  do  not  participate  in  the  actively 
conducted  action  potential. 

Interneurons 

It  is  convenient,  for  the  gross  identifications  possible 
in  the  spinal  cord  with  these  techniques,  to  define  an 
interneuron  as  a  postsynaptic  unit  which  does  not 
.send  its  axon  to  ventral  roots.  The  criteria  for  deciding 
if  a  unit  is  postsynaptic  are  the  latency  of  its  response 
and  whether  it  responds  with  more  than  one  impulse 
to  a  single  afferent  volley.  There  is  no  doubt  that  if  we 
accept  spontaneously  firing  units  which  cannot  be 
driven  by  the  electrical  stimuli  available  we  may  in- 
clude some  primary  afferents  from  distant  receptors 
as  interneurons,  but  most  of  these  can  be  eliminated 
bv  the  regularity  of  their  firing.'  Figure  1 1  shows  two 

'  For  a  discussion  of  possible  confusion  between  interneurons 
and  primary  afferents  conducting  dorsal  root  reHexes  see  Frank 
&  Fuortes  C26"). 


typical  units  satisfying  the  above  criteria.  Many  work- 
ers have  studied  patterns  of  activity  of  single  cells 
with  extracellular  electrodes  and  some  have  reported 
patterns  of  intracellular  potentials.  For  references  to 
these,  see  especially  Chapters  II  and  I\'  of  this 
volume  and  their  bibliographies. 

One  class  of  interneurons  deserves  mention  in  a  dis- 
cussion of  neuron  identification.  Occasionally  units 
are  penetrated  which  respond  to  a  single  ventral  root 
volley  with  a  very  high  frequency  train  of  spikes  in- 
stead of  the  single  action  potential  shown  by  moto- 
neurons. Eccles  et  at.  (19)  have  named  these  Renshaw 
cells  in  honor  of  Birdsey  Renshaw  (49)  who  predicted 
the  existance  of  such  cells  making  synapses  with  the 
axon  collaterals  of  motoneurons.  The  function  of  these 
cells  has  not  been  established,  but  Eccles  et  al.  (19)  be- 


20 


^/v  ?.?v:-i:^....^,/. 


■ 

^f^T^ 

- 

- 

/  ^ 
-// 

1    -; 

\:'-  ^f. 

1-2 
msec 


1-5 


1-8 


2-1 


FIG.  12.  Charts  illustrating  distribution  of  duration  of  spikes 
recorded  from  different  structures  in  the  cat's  spinal  cord. 
Abscissa:  spike  duration  in  msec;  ordinates:  number  of  spikes 
within  0.3  msec,  groups.  A.  Spikes  recorded  from  dorsal  and 
ventral  root  fibers.  B.  Spikes  recorded  from  motoneuron  somata 
or  dendrites  identified  by  criteria  given  in  text.  C.  Spikes  re- 
corded from  postsynaptic  elements  of  the  cord  other  than 
those  of  .1  and  B.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (26).] 


IDENTIFICATION    AND    ANALYSIS   OF  SINGLE   UNIT   ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL  NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


'^73 


lieve  that  they  supply  inhibition  to  the  motoneurons, 
the  activity  of  which  excites  them. 

That  both  fibers  and  somata  of  interneurons  are 
penetrated  is  indicated  by  figure  12  which  shows  that, 
after  known  primary  afferents  (^A)  and  motoneuron 
somata  (5)  are  eliminated,  the  remaining  spikes  from 
interneurons  are  distributed  in  duration  of  action 
potentials  as  though  they  were  made  up  of  many 
fibers  and  fewer  somata. 

Slow  PdtnUiali 

As  has  been  shown  above  it  has  not  been  possible 
yet  to  distinguish  clearly  between  cell  somata  and 
dendrites  with  intracellular  electrodes.  But  grouping 
these  two  structures  together  a  fairly  clear-cut  distinc- 
tion is  possible  between  soma-dendrites  and  fibers  on 
the  basis  of  the  slow  potentials  recorded  from  within 
them.  As  seen  in  figure  13,  a  unit  classified  as  a  moto- 
neuron soma  shows  a  long  lasting  graded  response  to 
subthreshold  excitatory  afiTerent  stimulation  which  is 
commonly  called  a  synaptic  potential.  If  this  potential 
reaches  a  critical  level  of  depolarization,  as  shown  in 
figure  14,  a  spike  is  initiated  and  this  spike  is  followed 


->  B 


f^ 


5  nnec 


lOmV 


-V- 


FIG.  13.  Spikes  and  slow  potentials  recorded  from  a  cat's 
motoneuron  following  antidromic  (o)  and  orthodromic  (0) 
stimulation.  .All  records  made  from  same  motoneuron  at  difler- 
ent  sweep  speeds  and  amplifications.  Note  respective  time  and 
potential  scales,  msec,  marks  shown  in  A,  B  and  D.  Note  dorsal 
root  spike  records  in  C  and  D  which  are  recorded  with  nega- 
tivity downward.  [From  Brock  et  al.  (i  i).] 


A 

0 

Outside  Potential 

11       :  "" 

L 

J 

■ 

-  Vs 

—  Vth 
— -Vm 

j,^ 

J  — 

' 

FIG.  14.  Diagram  illustrating  terminology  used  to  describe 
antidromic  (a)  and  orthodromic  (0)  spikes  from  cat's  moto- 
neurons and  the  level  of  polarization,  Vth,  which  must  be 
achieved  if  a  propagated  spike  is  initiated.  All  potentials  are 
measured  from  the  outside  potential  taken  as  o.  Vm,  resting 
membrane  potential,  inside  negative;  Vs,  total  spike  height; 
Vov,  spike  overshoot,  amount  inside  goes  positive  at  peak  of 
spike.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes  (27).] 


FIG.    15.    Spike    and   slow    potential    recorded    from    cat's 
motoneuron    following    antidromic    stimulation.    Calibration : 

20  mv.  Time:  i  msec. 


by  an  even  longer  lasting  period  of  hyperpolariza- 
tion,  such  as  those  in  figures  1 5  and  1 3C.  These  slow 
potentials  are  apparently  attentuated  in  the  axon  so 
that,  at  the  gains  employed  with  intracellular  record- 
ing, no  slow  potentials  are  recorded  from  fibers  unless 
they  are  penetrated  close  to  the  soma  (18,  26).  This 
finding,  which  is  based  upon  identifications  made 
using  the  criteria  just  discussed,  itself  becomes  a 
method  of  identifying  long  duration  spikes  as  from 
somata  or  dendrites  and  short  spikes  as  from  fibers. 


Steps  in  Development  of  Cell  Spikes 

Once  the  identification  of  the  various  units  pene- 
trated in  the  central  nervous  system  has  been  accepted. 


274 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY'    1 


a  further  study  can  be  made  of  the  details  of  their 
behavior.  The  most  definitive  of  these  studies  has 
been  made  on  the  large  motoneurons  of  the  cat  by 
Eccles  (i8),  Fuortes  et  al.  (29)  and  Fatt  (23),  and  in 
the  toad  by  Araki  et  al.  (9),  and  Araki  &  Otani  (8). 
While  these  studies  are  too  detailed  to  report  in  full, 
it  would  seem  pertinent  to  this  discussion  of  the 
identification  of  single  unit  activity  to  descrilje  briefly 
the  model  which  has  been  developed  to  account  for 
the  activity  of  certain  nerve  cells  (fig.  16).  All  nerve 
cells  certainly  do  not  behave  according  to  this 
model,  but  some  features  of  the  electrical  activity  of 
cells  are  so  general  that  they  justify  some  generaliza- 
tions from  a  model  Ijuilt  for  a  particular  cell. 

A  neuron  in  the  central  nervous  system  might  be 


NORMAL  THRESHOID 


FIG.  16.  Diagram  to  illustrate  initiation  of  impulses  in  a 
motoneuron.  A  spherical  soma  is  indicated  and  its  axis  passing 
through  the  center  of  the  axon  is  used  as  abscissa  for  the  plot. 
Ordinate:  membrane  depolarization  mejisured  from  resting 
membrane  potential.  The  dashed  line  indicates  relative  po- 
tential changes  evoked  in  various  positions  of  neuron  by 
synaptic  activity  in  soma  and  dendrites  (not  differentiated)  or 
by  currents  applied  through  a  microelectrode  in  the  sphere. 
The  solid  line  represents  the  depolarization  level  required  to 
evoke  firing  of  different  parts  of  the  membrane.  B'  designates 
that  part  of  the  neuron  having  a  high  threshold,  and  'A'  the 
transitional  region  between  this  area  and  the  lovi'  threshold 
axon.  In  normal  conditions  threshold  stimuli,  either  synaptic 
or  applied  through  microelectrode,  will  initiate  an  impulse  in  a 
region  where  the  dashed  line  first  crosses  above  the  solid  line 
(If  depolcirization  is  sudden,  attenuation  along  the  axon  will  be 
steeper  than  that  for  steady  depolarization  due  to  capacity  of 
the  membrane.)  The  dotted  line  is  intended  to  indicate  the 
depolarization  required  to  elicit  firing  shortly  after  activity 
which  has  involved  only  the  cross-hatched  areas  of  the  neuron, 
e.g.  after  a  blocked  antidromic  impulse.  [From  Fuortes  et  al. 
(29)J 


considered  to  consist  of  its  soma  and  dendrites  which 
are  connected  to  its  axon  by  a  thin  unmyelinated  seg- 
ment arising  from  the  axon  hillock.  Presynaptic  fibers 
make  synaptic  connections  with  this  cell  through 
terminal  knobs  which  end  on  its  soma  and  dendrites. 
Some  of  these  specialized  endings  are  excitatory  and 
others  inhil)it  activity  of  the  cell.  Probably  through  a 
mechanism  of  secretion  of  excitor  or  inhibitor  trans- 
mitter substances  by  the  presynaptic  terminals,  the 
membrane  of  the  postsynaptic  cell  is  made  selectively 
permealjle  to  certain  inorganic  ions  (16).  Normally 
these  transient  changes  in  ion  permeability  alter  the 
equilibriiuii  potential  of  the  membrane  and  either 
depolarize  it  (excitation)  or  hyperpolarize  it  (inhibi- 
tion). The  sum  of  these  synaptic  potentials  in  the  soma 
and  dendrites  spreads  electrotonically  with  decrement 
to  a  sensitive  target  area,  probably  the  thin  initial  seg- 
ment of  the  axon  and  part  of  the  axon  hillock.  The 
threshold  of  this  region,  that  is  the  magnitude  of  the 
depolarization  necessary  to  start  an  action  potential  in 
it,  is  lower  here  than  in  the  soma  and  dendrites 
(normally  perhaps  one  third  l  Thus,  in  spite  of  the 
fact  that  the  synaptic  potential  is  larger  in  the  soma 
and  dendrites  than  in  the  thin  segment,  the  lower 
threshold  of  this  region  permits  it  to  be  the  site  of 
origin  of  the  propagated  action  potential.  The  action 
potential  is  then  propagated  out  the  axon  and  may  or 
may  not  spread  Ijackwards  over  the  soma  and  den- 
drites. Following  activity  the  soma-dendritic  region 
remains  refractory  longer  than  the  target  area  so  that 
a  second  spike  elicited  during  this  period  may  be  con- 
ducted in  the  axon  without  spreading  to  the  soma  or 
dendrites  as  seen  in  figure  lo.-l  and  in  the  paper  by 
Fuortes  et  al.  (29).  It  is  proljable  that  the  safety  factor 
for  propagation  from  target  area  to  soma-dendritic 
region  varies,  not  only  with  the  condition  of  the  cell, 
but  also  from  one  type  of  cell  to  another  so  that 
normal  ijehavior  in  cells  may  differ  in  this  respect. 
But  cells  in  so  many  different  parts  of  the  nervous 
system  show  similar  electrical  properties  that  the 
main  features  of  the  model  just  described  may  well  be 
of  general  application. 

Stimulation  Through  Microelectrodes 

Electric  currents  delivered  through  intracellular 
microelectrodes  produce  excitation  similar  to  that 
caused  by  conducted  impulses.  Such  direct  stimula- 
tion plays  a  role  in  the  identification  of  penetrated 
units,  and  is  so  generally  useful  in  studying  the  proper- 
ties of  cells  that  some  reference  should  be  made  to  the 


IDENTIFICATION    AND    ANALYSIS  OF  SINGLE   UNIT   ACTIVITY   IN    CENTRAL   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


275 


to 
Ampliher 


Fig.l  9 


FIG.  I7..-1.  A  double-barrelled  microelectrode  and  its  immediate  connections.  Typical  values  are 
given  of  the  several  electrical  characteristics  which  are  significant  in  the  use  of  the  electrode.  B. 
Enlarged  view  showing  approximate  equivalent  circuit  with  motoneuron  ignoring  reactance. 
[From  Coombs  el  al.  (14).] 

FIG.  18.  Diagram  of  arrangement  for  recording  the  response  of  a  motoneuron  to  rectangular  current 
pulse  delivered  through  an  intracellular  electrode.  MN,  motoneuron;  Rp,  microelectrode  resistance; 
R/,  resistance  of  spinal  cord  and  bath;  b,  unit  dry  cell;  r,  50012  (6  and  r  compensate  for  membrane 
and  electrode  potentials);  R2,  2  kO;  r',  2000;  R,,  98.3  Mil;  R/,  0.92  MU.  Rectangular  pulses  applied 
between  E  and  D.  Spike  potentials  recorded  from  A  and  C.  Current  through  electrode  was  moni- 
tored by  recording  potential  across  R,'.  [From  Araki  &  Otani  (8).] 

FIG.  ig.  Two  arrangements  used  for  stimulating  and  recording  through  a  single  micropipette 
electrode.  R, :  i  kit;  R2:  10  kQ;  Rj:  44MS2.  Electrode  resistance  R,.  usually  between  10  and  100  Mil. 
The  resistor  of  the  calibrator  (Cal)  and  the  variable  resistor  of  the  compensator  (Comp),  lOoO  each. 
The  fi.xed  resistor  of  the  compensator  300S2  and  the  battery  supplies  1.5  v.  Stimulating  and  cali- 
brating pulses  are  applied  through  radio-frequency  stimulus  isolation  units.  The  indiflTerent  electrode, 
I.E.,  is  a  silver -silverchloride  wire  and  is  usually  placed  in  the  cat's  mouth.  Sw  is  a  switch  used  for 
d.c.  compensation  and  for  measurement  of  Re.  In  A  stimulating  current  is  measured  by  the  voltage 
drop  across  R2,  which  is  equal  to  the  drop  across  R3  when  the  bridge  is  balanced.  B  shows  an  alterna- 
tive method  for  measuring  current.  R.  and  R3  are  same  as  in  A.  R,  has  a  value  of  5  Ml2,  and  two 
preamplifiers  of  balanced  gain  are  used  for  differential  recording  of  the  voltage  drop  across  Rj. 
CRO  is  a  double-beam  oscilloscope  indicating  electrode  current  and  voltage.  [From  Frank  &  Fuortes 
(27).] 


276 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOOV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


two  main  techniques  which  have  been  used.  Coombs 
et  al.  (14)  used  a  double-barrelled  micropipette  (fis;. 
17)  passing  stimulating  or  polarizing  currents  through 
one  barrel  while  recording  the  intracellular  potential 
with  the  other  (barrel.  Araki  &  Otani  (8)  (fig.  18)  and 
Frank  &  Fuortes  (27)  (fig.  19)  used  a  bridge  circuit 
to  penuit  simultaneous  stimulation  and  recording 
through  the  same  microelectrode  tip.  The.se  articles 
should  be  consulted  for  details  and  limitations  of  the 
two  techniques. 

By  means  of  these  techniques  it  has  been  possible  to 
study  the  excitabilities  of  penetrated  units.  Differen- 
tiation of  axons  from  .somata  is  often  possible  on  the 
basis  of  their  excitabilities.  For  example  large  axons  in 
the  cat's  spinal  cord  have  a  rheobasic  current  of 
about  1.7  X  I  o~' ainp.,  while  units  identified  as  moto- 
neuron  somata   or  dendrites   require  an   average  of 


about  7   X    io~'  amp.   through  the  micropipette  to 
reach  threshold. 

Cells  firing  with  regular  trains  of  impulses  appear 
to  generate  their  own  impulses  at  some  particular  site 
of  origin.  When  depolarizing  currents  are  applied 
through  the  micropipette  placed  near  such  a  locus  the 
rate  of  firing  is  increased  in  proportion  to  the  applied 
current.  Since  the  applied  current  decrements  rapidly 
along  an  axon  and  presumably  also  along  a  dendrite, 
current  through  the  pipette  will  not  affect  the  firing 
rate  when  the  micropipette  is  at  a  distance  from  this 
site  of  origin.  It  is  therefore  possible  to  tell  whether 
the  locus  of  recurrent  firing  in  a  cell  is  near  to  or  far 
from  the  tip  of  the  micropipette.  If  it  is  accepted  that 
such  a  locus  is  normally  situated  near  the  axon  hil- 
lock, then  the  response  of  a  repetitively  firing  unit  to 
applied  polarizing  currents  can  be  used  to  infer 
whether  it  is  an  axon  or  a  soma. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Adri.'^n,  E.  D.  .'\nd  D.  VV.  Bronk.  J.  Ph\siol.  67:  1 19,  1929.  24 

2.  Adrian,   E.   D.   and  Y.   Zotterman.  J.   Physiol.   61  :    151, 

1926.  25, 

3.  Adrian,  R.  H.  J.  Physiol.  133:  631,  1956.  26, 

4.  Alexander,  J.  T.  and  W.   L.  Nastuk.   Rev.  Scient.  Inslru-          27. 
mcn(j  24:  528,  1953.  28. 

5.  Altamirano,    M.,    C.    W.    Coaxes   and    H.  Grundfest.          29. 
J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38:  319,  1955. 

6.  Alvord,  E.  C,  Jr.  and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes.  J.  Physiol.  122:  30. 

302,  1953- 

7.  Amassian,   V.    E.   Eleclroencefihalog.  &   Clin,   .\emophysiol.   5:  31. 

415.  '953-  3^- 

8.  .Araki,  T.  and  T.  Otani.  J.  Neurophr.uol .  18:  472,  1955. 

9.  Araki,  T.,T.  OT.'kNi  .\nd  T.  FuRtjKAW.\.  Jup.  J.  Physiol.  3:  33. 

254.  '953- 

10.  Barron,  D.  H.  and  B.  H.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  94:  26,  34. 

■938. 

11.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol  35. 

"7:43'.  1952- 

12.  Bullock,  T.  H.  and  C.  Terzuolo.  J.  Physiol.    138:  341,  36. 

■957-  37- 

13.  Cohen,  J.   J.,    S.    Hagiwara    and    Y.    Zotterman.  Ada          38. 
physiol.  scandinav.  33:  316,  1955.  39. 

14.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.  130: 

291,  1955.  40. 

15.  CoOMBS,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.   130: 

326,  1955.  41. 

16.  DowBEN,  M.  AND  J.  E.  RosE.  Science  1  18:  22,  1953.  42. 

17.  del  Castillo,  J.  and  B.  Katz.  J.  Physiol.  128:  396,  1955. 

18.  Eccles,  J.  C.J.  Physiol.  139:  198,  1957.  43. 

19.  Eccles,  J.  C,  P.  Fatt  and  K.  Koketsu.  J.  Physiol.   126: 

524.  1954-  44- 

20.  Eccles,  J.  C,  P.  Fatt  and  S.  Landcren.  J.  .\'europhysiol. 

'9:  75.  1957-  45- 

21.  Eccles,  J.  C.  and  H.  E.  Hoff.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  46. 
no:  483,  1932.  47. 

22.  Fatt,  P.  J.  Neurophysiol .  20:  27,  1957. 

23.  Fatt,  P.  J.  Neurophysiol.  20:  61,  1957.  48. 


Fessard,  .\.  .\ND  B.  H.  C.   M.atthews.  J.  Physiol.  95:  39, 

'939- 

Frank,  K.  .\I.\  Internal.  Physiol.  Congr.:  362,  1953. 

Frank,  K.  and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes.  J.  Physiol.  130:  625,  1955. 

Frank,  K.  and  M.  G.  F.  Fuortes,  J.  Physiol.  134:  451,  1956. 

Frevgang,  W.  H.,  Jr.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  41 :  543,  1958 

Fuortes,  M.  G.  F.,  K.  Frank  and  M.  C.  Becker.  J.  Gen. 

Physiol.  40:  735,  1957. 

Graham,  J.  and  R.  \V.  Gerard.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol. 

28:  99,  1946. 

Granit,  R.  and  G.  Strom.  J.  Neurophysiol.  14:  113,  1951. 

Grundfest,  H.,  R.  VV.  Sengtaken,  W.  H.  Oettinger  and 

R.  W.  Gurrv.  Rev.  Scient.  Instruments  21  :  360,  1950. 

Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  E.  F.  MacNichol, 

Jr.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Quant.  Biol.  17:  125,  1952. 

Hess,  W.  R.  Beitrage  zur  Physiologie  des  Hirnslammes.  Liep- 

zig:  Thieme,   1932. 

Howland,   B.,  J.   Y.   Lettvin,   VV.  S.   McCulloch,    VV. 

Pitts  and  P.  D.  Wall.  J.  Physiol.  122:  24P,  1953. 

HuBEL,  D.  H.  Science  125;  549,  1957. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  J.  Physiol.  1 17:  359,  1952. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38:  813,  1955. 

Jenerick,  H.   p.  .and  R.  W.  Gerard.  J.   Cell,  (s    Comp. 

Physiol.  42  '.  79,    1 953. 

Kato,  G.,   Z.   Kaku  .and  I.  Tasaki.   W  Inlcrnat.  Physiol. 

Congr. :  95,   1 935. 

Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  17:  558,  1934. 

Ling,  G.  and  R.  W.  Gerard.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  34: 

383,  1949. 

Livingston,  L.  G.  and  B.  M.  Dugcar.  Biol.  Bull.  67 :  504, 

■934- 

Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  and  A.  K.  McIntvre.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38: 

771.  '955- 

Marshall,  W.  H.  Stain  Technol.  15:  133,  1940. 

Nastuk,  VV.  L.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  42:  249,  1953. 

Nastuk,   W.    L.    and   A.    L.    Hodgkin.   J.    Cell.  &   Comp. 

Physiol.  35:  39,  1950. 

Raiport,  M.  Fed.  Proc.   16:  104,   1957. 


IDENTIFICATION   AND   ANALYSIS   OF  SINGLE   UNIT  ACTIVITY   IN   CENTRAL   NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


277 


49.  Renshaw,  B.  J.  Neurophysiol.  9;  191,  1946. 

50.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle.  Bull,  Jolins  Hopkins 
Hasp.  94:  238,  1954. 

SoLMs,  S.  J.,  \V.  L.  Nastuk  and  J.  T.  Alexander.  Rev. 
Sclent.  Instruments  24:  960,   1953- 
SvAETiCHiN,  G.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  suppl.  86,  24:  5,  1951. 

53.  SvAETicniN,  G.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  suppl.  86,  24:  278, 

'95'- 

54.  Tasaki,  I.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  3:  73,  1952. 


51 


52 


55.  Tasaki,  I.,  E.  H.  Polley  ahd  F.  Orrego.  J.  .Neurophysiol. 

17:454.  1954- 

56.  ToENNIES,  J.  F.J.  \europhysiol.  i  :  378,  1938. 

57.  Wagner,  H.  G.  and  E.  F.  MacNichol,  Jr.  Navy  Med.  Res. 
Inst.  Project  Report  12:  97,  1954. 

58.  Woodbury,  J.  W.  Fed.  Proc.  12:  159,  1953. 

59.  Woodbury,  J.  W.  and  A.J.  Brady.  Science  123:  100,  1956. 

60.  WooDBUR'i',  J.  W.  AND  H.  D.  Patton.  Cold  Spring  Harbor 
Symp.  Quanl.  Biol.  17:  185,  1952. 


CHAPTER    XI 


Intrinsic  rhythms  of  the  brain 


W  .    GREY    W  ALTER      '     Burden  Neurological  Institute,  Bristol,  England 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Generation  of  Spontaneous  Oscillation 

Piesalence  of  Spontaneous  Rhythms 

Origin  of  Spontaneous  Activity 

Conditions  for  Oscillation 
Simple  harmonic  motion 
Relaxation  oscillators 
Distinction    between    simple    harmonic    and    relaxation 

oscillators 
Electrical  equivalent  of  hydraulic  model 
Spontaneous  Electrical  Activity  in  Excitable  Tissues 

Rhythmic  Activity  in  Single  Units 

Rhythmic  Activity  in  Networks 

Rhythmic  Activity  in  Primitive  Organs 

Intrinsic  Rhythms  in  the  Human  Brain 
Properties  of  Alpha  Activity  as  Typical  of  Intrinsic  Rhythms 

Early  Reports 

Individuality  of  Alpha  Rhythms  and  I'heir  Variation 

Complexity  of  Alpha  Rhythms 

Identification  of  Alpha  Components 

Degree   of  Constancy    and    Range   of   Variation    in    Alpha 
Frequency 

Effects  of  Activation  and  Stimulation 

Synchronization  of  Alpha  Rhythms 

Evidence  from  Intracerebral  Electrodes 

Relation  Between  Alpha  Rhythms  and  Effector  Function 

Effect  of  Temperature  Changes 
Delta,  Theta  and  Beta  Rhythms 

Relation  of  Delta  and  Theta  Rhythms  to  Age 

Delta  Rhythms 

Theta  Rhythms 

Beta  Rhythms 
Origin  of  Intrinsic  Rhythms 


GENERATION    OF   SPONTANEOUS    OSCILLATION 


Prevalence  oj  Spontaneous  Rhythms 

DURING  THE  30  YEARS  that  have  elapsed  since  Berger 
first  bcffan  to  record  electrical  acti\it\-  from  human 


brains,  many  suggestions  ha\e  been  made  to  account 
for  the  unexpected  spontaneity  and  regularity  of  these 
rhythmic  potential  changes  which  resemble  so  little 
the  familiar  action  potentials  of  the  peripheral  nerve. 
Rhythmic  electrochemical  activity  is  not  in  itself  a 
rare  phenomenon;  it  is  common  in  primitive  or- 
ganisms and  can  appear  even  in  simple  inorganic 
chemical  reactions  such  as  that  between  iron  and 
nitric  acid  or  between  mercury  and  hydrogen  perox- 
ide (39).  Such  reactions,  of  course,  involve  more  than  a 
simple  combination  of  forces  or  reagents;  for  the 
generation  of  rhythmic  actixity  there  must  always  be 
present  in  the  system  some  sort  of  circular  or  feed-back 
pathway  through  which  the  effect  of  products  of 
the  reaction  can  influence  the  state  of  the  original 
reagents. 

Origin  of  Spontaneous  Activity 

In  such  a  system  where  action  and  reaction  are 
intercoupled,  activity  once  initiated  will  tend  to 
persist,  but  the  first  cause  may  be  obscure. '  Spontane- 
ous' acti\ity  is  in  fact  a  difficult  conception  to  define 
or  illustrate  in  practice  and  the  situation  is  not 
simplified  by  substituting  the  terms  'endogenous', 
'autogenous'  or 'autochthonous',  for  in  all  these  words 
there  is  iinplicit  the  assumption  that  the  behavior  of 
the  system  depends  not  on  its  previous  state  but  in 
some  way  on  itself,  as  if  there  were  an  element  of 
choice  or  free  will.  This  implication  is  verbal  rather 
than  philosophical  and  need  not  be  taken  very  seri- 
ously; the  difficulty  is  mainly  that  man-inade  ma- 
chines are  designed  for  obedience  rather  than  for  origi- 
nality and  it  is  difficult  to  define  the  use  of  function  of  a 
mechanism  that  seems  to  act  independently  of  outside 
influences.  Clearly,  if  the  electrical  rhythins  of  the 
brain  were  entirely  spontaneous  and  independent 
they  would  be  very  hard  to  fit  into  any  hypothesis  of 


279 


28o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


neurophysiology,  but,  as  is  well  known,  these  rhythmic 
discharges  are  generally  greatly  affected  by  external 
signals  and  need  be  considered  as  spontaneous  only 
in  the  sense  that  the  energy  required  for  their  main- 
tenance is  freely  available  in  the  brain  and  that  their 
existence  implies  some  sort  of  regenerative,  retroactive 
or  feed-back  loop.  Even  if,  as  several  observers  have 
suggested  (13,  21),  the  source  of  the  rhythmicity  may 
be  in  the  nerve  cells  themselves  rather  than  in  the 
manner  of  their  interconnection,  there  must  still  exist, 
even  within  this  intimate  microcosm,  a  re-entrant 
loop  of  energy  transfer  around  which  two  sets  of 
variables  can  mutually  control  one  another.  The 
physiological  nature  of  the  retroactive  pathway  is 
hard  to  identify  and  around  this  point  controversy 
has  raged  for  many  years,  involving  many  experiments 
and  strong  feelings.  Viewed  without  rancor,  the 
dispute  seems  academic;  most  of  the  claims  and  asser- 
tions on  both  sides  can  be  justified,  few  of  the  denials 
can  be  confirmed.  There  seems  little  doubt  that  in 
certain  circumstances  single  nerve  cells  can  discharge 
spontaneously  at  a  steady  rate  (3,  51).  On  the  other 
hand,  large  populations  of  healthy  isolated  brain 
cells  may  remain  quite  inactive  for  long  periods  (17) 
yet  respond  rhythmically  when  stimulated. 

Conditions  Jor  Oscillation 

SIMPLE  H.\RMONic  MOTION.  It  Can  be  shown  that,  when- 
ever and  wherever  an  oscillation  appears,  there  must 
be  a  retroactive  mechanism  of  some  kind.  The  most 
familiar — though  not  perhaps  most  strictly  relevant — 
form  of  oscillation  is  the  simple  harmonic  motion  of  a 
pendulum.  Even  in  the  case  of  the  simple  pendulum, 
sustained  vibration  depends  on  the  regular  transmu- 
tations of  position  and  velocity  as  shown  in  the  basic 
equations- 


dS 
dt 

dV 

~dt 


=   V, 


sin  5' 


where  ^S'  is  angular  position;  I",  velocity;  g,  constant  of 
gravitation;  and  L,  length  of  pendulum 

From  these  it  is  seen  that  changes  in  6'  depend  on 
the  value  of  V  while  changes  in  V  depend  on  the  value 
of  S.  This  is  the  basic  condition  for  feedback,  and 
wherever  two  variables  are  thus  interdependent 
oscillatory  behavior  is  likely  to  occur.  From  the 
dynamic  standpoint  a  swinging  pendulum  is  not  a 
single  object  but  a  system  of  two  variables.  Whether 


the  system  will  be  '  spontaneously'  active  or  stable  is 
another  question  and  depends  upon  the  sign  of  the 
constants  that  determine  the  feed-back  ratio.  In  the 
case  of  the  pendulum,  the  sign  is  negative,  so  the 
system  is  stable  near  its  resting  state.  An  "ideal' 
frictionless  penduliun  however  would  be  unstable 
because  the  random  Brownian  movement  of  its 
molecules  would  inevitably  set  up  an  oscillation  at  its 
natural  frequency  which,  in  the  absence  of  damping, 
would  continue  indefinitely.  This  effect  can  be  ob- 
served in  the  case  of  the  very  small  light  suspensions 
of  sensitive  galvanometers. 

A  large  pendulum,  however,  is  stable  in  the  sense 
that  the  frictional  losses  limit  the  extent  and  duration 
of  any  o.scillation.  In  the  'ideal'  case  the  feed-back 
factor  is  unity;  in  any  practical  case  it  is  less  than 
unity,  so  to  sustain  an  oscillation  energy  must  be 
supplied  from  outside  the  system.  Furthermore,  the 
more  massive  the  system,  the  more  precisely  must  the 
energv  be  distributed  in  tiine  so  as  to  reinforce  the 
movement  of  the  pendulum;  it  must  be  phase-locked 
to  the  oscillatory  element. 

This  example  of  simple  harmonic  motion  illustrates 
two  ways  in  which  rhythmic  activity  may  be  gen- 
erated: first  by  interaction  between  a  'noisy'  or  random 
energy  source  and  a  small-scale  or  loss-free  retroactive 
system,  second  by  interaction  between  a  phase-con- 
trolled energy  source  and  a  normally  damped  retro- 
active system.  Obviously  intermediate  conditions 
between  these  extremes  exist  and  many  of  them  can 
give  the  impression  of  '  spontaneity'  because  the 
relation  between  the  time-distribution  of  energy  and 
the  degree  of  damping  of  the  oscillatory  system  may 
not  be  obvious  without  careful  experiment.  In  such 
a  system,  activity  once  initiated  will  tend  to  persist, 
but  the  first  cause  may  be  obscure. 

RELAXATION  osciLL.\TORS.  Lcst  it  sliould  be  thought 
that  some  sort  of  simple  harmonic  motion  is  the  only 
possible  source  of  rhythmic  activity,  another  mechani- 
cal illustration  should  be  considered.  This  is  in  the 
class  of 'relaxation  oscillation";  a  simple  example  is  the 
autosiphon  in  which  one  end  of  an  inverted  U  tube  is 
connected  to  the  outlet  of  a  water  tank  with  the  top  of 
the  n  below  the  top  of  the  tank  and  the  open  end  of 
the  n  near  the  bottom  of  the  tank.  If  the  tank  be  now 
filled  from  a  steady  source,  the  level  will  rise  in  a 
linear  fashion  until  the  water  level  reaches  the  top  of 
the  n .  At  this  point  a  siphon  will  be  formed  and  the 
water  tank  will  empty  through  the  siphon,  if  the  rate  of 
flow  through  the  fl  is  greater  than  that  from  the 
source.    When    the   water  level   falls   below   the  open 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


281 


end  of  the  pipe,  the  siphon  will  be  broken  and  the 
whole  process  will  be  repeated.  The  rate  of  change  of 
water  level  with  respect  to  time  o\er  the  cycle  will 
approximate  to  two  intersectine;  straight  lines,  the 
slope  of  which  will  depend  upon  the  rate  of  filling  and 
the  rate  of  emptying  respectively.  An  interesting  case 
is  when  the  rate  at  which  the  siphon  empties  is  exactly 
equal  to  the  rate  of  filling;  in  these  circumstances  the 
water  level  will  rise  linearly  to  a  maximum  and  remain 
there  indefinitely.  The  system  is  stable,  but  the  flow 
of  water  is  continuous.  Obviously,  in  such  conditions, 
a  slight  change  in  flow  rate  at  input  or  output  will 
engender  relaxation  oscillations  of  level.  Instability 
in  this  system  will  result  from  a  rise  or  fall  in  input  or 
output,  and  only  very  careful  examination  of  the 
rate  chart  would  disclose  which,  in  any  particular 
occasion,  was  the  most  likely  cause. 

Another  interesting  feature  of  this  system  is  that  a 
transient  change  in,  say,  the  rate  of  water  input  can 
act  as  a  "  stimulus'  to  initiate  a  complete  cycle  of 
operation  provided  the  rate  of  change  exceeds  a 
certain  threshold.  The  value,  form  and  scale  of  the 
response  to  such  a  'stimulus'  will  be  independent  of 
the  nature  of  the  stimulus  and  will  be  all-or-none.  It 
will  also  have  an  absolute  and  a  relative  refractory 
period.  In  fact,  a  system  of  this  type  is  closely  anal- 
ogous to  the  schema  generally  proposed  for  nervous 
action.  The  steady  filling  and  emptying  of  the  tank 
corresponds  with  the  metabolism  of  an  excitable 
structure,  and  the  excitability-stability  relation  is 
similarly  dependent  on  the  maintenance  of  a  dynamic 
equilibrium.  Furthermore,  there  is  illustrated  a  rela- 
tion between  excitability  and  homeostasis.  If  the 
constancy  of  water  level  in  the  tank — or  potential 
diflference  in  a  nerve  cell — is  regarded  as  an  important 
condition,  then  the  system  is  evidently  an  admirable 
device  for  autoregulation  within  certain  limits  of 
external  variation.  The  standard  unit  '  response'  of 
discharge — and  replenishment — is  a  signal  that  the 
limits  of  self  control  have  been  exceeded.  Continued 
rhythmic  activity  is  a  signal  of  sustained  excess  or 
deficiency. 

DISTINCTION  BErVVEEN  SIMPLE  H.^RMONIC  .AND  RELAXA- 
TION OSCILLATORS.  The  behavior  of  this  elementary 
hydraulic  model  may  be  contrasted  with  the  simple 
harmonic  motion  of  a  pendulum;  the  waveform  in  the 
first  case  is  a  series  of  asymmetric  transients  while  in 
the  second  it  is  of  course  strictly  sinusoidal.  The  auto- 
siphon  shows  no  tendency  to  oscillate  after  a  dis- 
turbance is  over  whereas  the  pendulum  exhibits  a 
damped  train  of  vibrations.  Similarly,  the  autosiphon. 


when  stable,  can  be  triggered  to  give  a  full  cycle  of 
activity  by  a  minimal  but  supraliminal  stimulus, 
whereas  the  pendulum  requires  either  full  scale  tran- 
sient deflection  or  repeated  stimulation  at  its  natural 
frequency  to  evoke  a  maximal  discharge.  The  res- 
onance of  the  pendulum  is  typical  of  such  systems; 
the  strict  relation  between  sharpness  of  resonance  and 
length  of  build-up  and  die-away  time  is  important. 
The  autosiphon  exhibits  no  true  resonance;  its 
response  is  all-or-none.  However,  it  can  display  the 
phenomenon  of  pararesonance;  the  maximum  rate  of 
discharge  is  produced  most  economically  when 
stimuli  are  given  at  the  same  rate  as  the  natural  period 
of  the  operation  cycle. 

ELECTRIC     EQUIVALENT     OF     HYDRAULIC     MODEL.     This 

detailed  analogy  is  presented  because  appreciation  of 
the  differences  between  the  two  main  classes  of 
rhythmic  activity  is  essential  for  understanding  the 
difliculties  which  still  surround  interpretation  of  the 
rhythmic  electrical  phenomena  in  the  nervous  system. 

The  character  of  the  relaxation  oscillator  which  is 
most  instructive  physiologically  is  that  it  is  a  nonlinear 
system;  its  operation  depends  upon  the  sharp  thresh- 
old which  separates  one  regime  from  another. 

If  the  hydraulic  model  seems  too  trivial,  the  com- 
ponents may  be  replaced  with  electrical  ones,  po- 
tential difference  for  water-level,  current  for  flow, 
capacitors  for  the  tank,  resistors  for  the  pipes,  dis- 
charge tubes  with  their  nonlinear  voltage-current 
characteristics  for  the  siphon.  This  produces  a  circuit 
arrangement  familiar  to  electronic  designers  as  a  time- 
base  or  sawtooth  oscillator.  In  fact,  .such  an  electric 
model  has  been  built  and  is  in  regular  use  for  teaching 
and  demonstration  to  illustrate  the  behavior  of  a 
system  containing  several  such  circuits  in  a  chain  or 
cascade  C56).  In  this  embodiment  of  the  analogue, 
a  series  of  such  systems,  coupled  together,  can  be  seen 
to  provide  for  propagation,  inhibition,  unidirectional 
synaptic  transmission  and  other  basic  properties  of 
axonic  and  neuronic  action.  When  more  elaborate 
forms  of  interconnection  and  switching  are  provided, 
properties  such  as  homeostasis  and  ultrastability 
appear,  as  demonstrated  by  the  machines  constructed 
by  Ashby  (4)  and  Uttley  (52). 

The  working  hypothesis  embodied  in  the  simple 
model  of  nervous  action  is  that  every  element  of  a 
nerve  cell  from  soma  to  terminal  dendrite  is,  in 
effect,  a  miniature  relaxation  oscillator.  Each  element 
thus  considered  is  connected  to  tho.se  on  both  sides  of 
it  so  as  to  facilitate  by  its  own  activity  their  tendency 
to  discharge.  A  crude  hydraulic  counterpart  would 


282 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


be  a  series  of  autosiphons,  each  emptying  into  the  one 
beside  it  so  that  should  any  one  element  operate,  the 
whole  chain  would  be  initiated.  This  model  would  not 
work,  however,  for  it  requires  that  each  element  be 
above  all  the  others  which  is  absurd.  In  the  electric 
model  this  difficulty  is  overcome  by  providing  capaci- 
tors which  direct  a  proportion  of  the  discharge  cur- 
rent of  each  element  to  the  trigger  tubes  of  the  ad- 
jacent ones  so  as  momentarily  to  raise  the  potential 
differences  across  them  to  their  discharge  threshold. 
This  is  an  important  feature,  since  it  suggests  that  in 
a  living  nerve  the  current  generated  at  any  active 
region,  the  action  current,  may  produce  fields  of  po- 
tential difference  great  enough  to  initiate  at  a  distance 
the  electrochemical  process  responsible  for  the  char- 
acteristic depolarization  and  discharge.  This  effect  is 
of  course  demonstrated  ijy  the  circumvention  of  a 
blocked  region  in  a  nerve  by  leading  the  action  cur- 
rent through  an  inert  conducting  bridge  (30). 


SPONTANEOUS    RHYTH.MIC    ACTIVITY    IN 
EXCITABLE    TISSUES 

Rhvthmic  Activity  in  Singh'  Units 

With  these  properties  in  mind  we  may  revert  to 
the  question  of  spontaneous  rhythmicity.  In  a  single 
nerve  cell,  as  in  a  single  clement  of  the  nerve  model, 
spontaneous  rhythmic  activity  will  tend  to  occur 
whenever  the  discharge  threshold  is  at  or  below  the 
polarization  potential.  As  is  well  known,  depression  of 
the  threshold  of  excitation,  or  lengthening  of  the  ac- 
commodation constant  by  the  action  of  drugs,  does 
induce  spontaneous  rhythmic  activity,  even  in 
normally  passive  peripheral  nerve  fibers  (19).  In  gen- 
eral, the  amplitude  and  rate  of  .such  a  discharge  de- 
pends on  two  factors:  the  rate  of  charge  or  polariza- 
tion, and  the  rate  of  discharge  or  depolarization. 
These  two  time  constants  are  independent  variables 
to  a  first  order  of  approximation  and  may  be  analo- 
gous to  the  two  resistors  in  a  sawtooth  relaxation 
oscillator  which  control  the  sweep  speed  and  fly-back 
speed,  respectively.  In  this  comparison  the  flyback  is 
equivalent  to  the  action  potential  or  spike  discharge, 
which  need  not  be  numerically  equal  to  the  total 
available  polarization  potential. 

Rliytlimic  Activity  in  .Netwurks 

Now,  when  several  such  elements  can  interact  wiih 
one   another   by   their  electric   fields,    the   aggregate 


system  will  tend  to  exhibit  generalized  rhythmic  ac- 
tivity at  a  frequency  very  much  lower  than  that  sug- 
gested by  the  time  constants  of  the  single  elements. 
The  repolarization  time  of  neurons  in  the  central 
nervous  system  is  probably  equivalent  to  their  re- 
fractory period  and  lasts  about  i  msec.  The  maximum 
frequency  of  spontaneous  discharge  for  such  a  neuron 
is  therefore  of  the  order  of  1000  pulses  per  sec,  but 
the  lowest  rate  depends  on  the  relation  of  the  degree 
of  depolarization  to  the  threshold.  In  effect  this  im- 
plies that  there  should  be  an  inverse  relation  between 
the  amplitude  and  the  frequency  of  a  spontaneous 
rhythm. 

The  i^asic  waveform  of  a  discharge  determined  in 
this  way  should  be  of  an  asymmetrical  sawtooth 
variety,  the  asymmetry  being  more  apparent  at 
higher  amplitude,  though  the  proportions  are  actually 
constant.  It  can  be  shown,  however,  that  in  the  case 
of  a  large  population  of  mutually  interacting  unstable 
elements  the  waveform  of  the  aggregate  discharge 
may  be  so  smoothed  as  to  lose  all  traces  of  its  angu- 
larity and  come  to  lie  indistinguishable  from  a 
sinusoidal  rhythm.  This  principle  is  actually  applied 
in  electronic  circuit  design  to  obtain  a  sine  wave  sig- 
nal from  a  square  or  triangular  source  which  can  be 
activated  or  synchronized  without  the  inertia  of  a 
conventional  sine  wave  oscillator. 

The  conclusion  to  be  drawn  is  that  the  wav-e  form 
of  a  spontaneous  rhythm  originating  in  a  population 
of  active  elements  is  of  limited  assistance  in  determin- 
ing the  mechanism  of  its  .source;  a  pure  sine  wave  may 
originate  in  an  assembly  of  relaxation  oscillators,  but 
a  relaxation  wave  form  is  less  likely  to  be  the  output 
of  a  single  harmonic  source. 


Rhytlimic  Activity  in  Piiinitive  Organs 

Having  now  considered  the  basic  properties  of 
rhythmic  generators  in  general,  we  may  turn  to  the 
specific  features  of  this  class  of  activity  in  the  brain. 
At  the  very  outset  it  must  be  admitted  that  no  con- 
venient generalization  is  possible.  Rhythmic  dis- 
charges are  common  in  the  nervous  and  muscular 
systems  of  nearly  all  animals,  but  there  is  as  yet  no 
proof  that  they  can  all  be  attributed  to  the  same 
mechanism. 

For  example,  details  of  the  intrinsic  rhythms  of  the 
cardiac  ganglion  cells  in  Crustacea  have  been  de- 
scribed by  Hagiwara  &  Bullock  {26)  and  Bullock  & 
Terzuolo  (14).  The  wave  form  of  the.se  rhythms 
seems  to  be  typical  of  the  relaxation  oscillator  type, 
as  shown  in  figure   i.   Harris   &   Whiting  {27)    hax'e 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS   OF    THE    BRAIN 


283 


FIG.  I.  Spontaneous  electrical  discharges  in  single  cells  of  lobster  cardiac  ganglion.  The  wave 
form  is  suggestive  of  a  relaxation  oscillation  vifith  two  separate  time  constants.  .4  and  B  are  from  the 
same  cell  in  the  lobster.  It  shows  a  large  pacemaker  potential,  presumably  arising  nearby,  and  an- 
other prepotential  before  the  spike.  This  can  fail  to  elicit  a  spike,  can  continue  (end  of  .-1)  or  redevelop 
(third  spike  of  Bj  after  the  spike,  and  can  initiate  repolarization  almost  as  complete  as  a  spike  can. 
Note  the  failure  of  the  prepotential  to  arise  following  the  third  spike  in  B,  with  instead  an  undulation 
leading  to  a  new  cycle.  C,  D  and  E  are  three  different  crab  cells  of  type  D,  showing  different  forms 
and  permutations  of  pacemaker  potential  and  repolarization.  Scales:  A,  B,  500  msec;  C,  D  and  E, 
50  mv,  200  msec.  [From  Bullock  &  Terzuolo  (14).] 


confirmed  the  early  olsservations  of  Pa  ton  (45)  and 
Wintrebert  (68)  that  in  embryonic  clasmobranchs 
spontaneous  rhythmic  activity  of  the  mu.sculature  is 
entirely  myogenic  in  origin.  A  tendency  to  repetitive 
activity  seems  to  be  intrinsic  in  many  excitable  struc- 
tures and,  indeed,  absence  of  spontaneous  discharges 
may  be  a  special  case  of  control  or  inhibition.  In 
primitive  or  embryonic  organisms  the  intrinsic 
rhythms  are  generally  little  affected  by  external 
stimuli,  whereas  in  more  highly  developed  structures, 
such  as  the  human  brain,  responsiveness  to  stimula- 
tion is  the  general  rule. 

This  difference  suggests  that,  as  in  other  living 
structures  and  functions,  a  basic  mechanism  which 
survived  the  first  stages  of  evolution  because  of  its 
simple  utility  may,  as  it  were,  be  exploited  in  the 
later  stages  of  specialization  to  fulfill  a  much  more 


elaborate  function.  As  a  crude  working  hypothesis,  we 
may  consider  that  the  rhythinic  properties  of  primi- 
tive creatures  and  rudimentary  organs,  which  at 
first  provided  a  simple  means  of  propulsion,  have  in 
our  own  brains  assumed  an  essential  role  in  the 
systematic  timing  and  distribution  of  information 
within  the  neuronic  network. 

Intrinsic  Rhythms  in  the  Human  Brain 

For  the  purpose  of  this  chapter,  attention  will  be 
directed  mainly  to  the  rhythmic  activity  of  the  human 
brain.  This  has  received  the  greatest  attention  since 
the  discovery  of  Hans  Berger  of  the  human  electro- 
encephalogram (9);  the  observation  and  measurement 
of  the  rhythmic  features  of  the  human  EEG  have  been 
practised  on  an  increasing  scale  for  20  years  in  several 


284 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


hundred  laboratories.  There  is  the  further  advantage 
that  such  correlation  with  function  as  may  be  estab- 
lished can  be  followed  more  closely  in  human  sub- 
jects whose  mentality  and  behavior  are  more  familiar 
and  comprehensible  than  are  those  of  other  animals. 
The  rhythmic  wave-like  potential  changes  desig- 
nated as  '  alpha  rhythms'  bv  Berger  are  the  most 
prominent  and  peculiar  feature  of  human  brain  ac- 
tivity, and  these  rhythms  will  be  taken  as  representa- 
tive of  rhythmic  activity  in  normal  conditions.  An 
example  appears  in  figure  2.  Since  Berger's  original 
discoveries,  brain  rhythms  have  been  subclassified, 
not  only  on  the  basis  of  their  frequency  and  ampli- 
tude but  also  on  their  provenance  and  functional 
correlation.  Employing  these  criteria,  three  other  main 
classes  of  rhythm  have  been  identified  in  human  sub- 
jects:'theta  rhythms'  with  a  frequency  of  4  to  7  cycles 
per  .sec,  occupying  typically  the  parietal  and  tem- 


poral regions  of  the  brain  and  associated  with  child- 
hood, and  emotional  stress  in  some  adults  (fig.  3); 
'delta  rhythms'  with  frequencies  from  less  than  i  up 
to  sM  cycles  per  sec,  associated  with  deep  sleep  in 
normal  adults,  with  infancy  and  with  organic  brain 
disease  (fig.  4);  and  'beta  rhythms'  with  frequencies 
higher  than  14  cycles  per  sec,  generally  associated 
with  activation  and  tension.  In  considering  the  nature 
and  correlations  of  the  alpha  rhythms,  it  should  always 
be  recalled  that  these  other  rhythmic  phenomena 
exist  and  that  their  mechanisms  may  be  as  different 
from  those  of  alpha  rhythms  as  are  their  functional 
associations.  Moreover,  in  the  realm  of  brain  pathol- 
ogy, relatively  enormous  rhythmic  discharges  are  as- 
sociated with  certain  types  of  epileptic  seizures,  and 
these  again  may  originate  in  a  manner  quite  different 
from  that  of  the  normal  alpha  rhythms. 


1001 1"  'i'jiV^ll,'^' 

FIG.  2.  An  example  of  the  classical  effect  of  eye  closure  on  alpha  rhythms  in  a  normal  subject. 
Upper  five  traces  were  recorded  from  electrode  sites  shown  in  the  diagram  in  the  upper  left  corner. 
These  five  primary  records  show  the  typical  burst  of  alpha  activity  as  the  eyes  are  closed,  followed  by 
marked  amplitude  modulation,  the  alpha  rhythms  being  most  prominent  in  the  posterior  occipital 
derivations.  The  sixth  trace  representing  the  frequency  analysis,  indicates  the  presence  of  two  com- 
ponents at  9  and  10  cycles  per  sec.  The  seventh  trace,  that  showing  the  period  analysis,  indicates 
the  presence  of  wave  intervals  varying  from  90  to  1 1  o  msec.  The  three  methods  of  display  are  es- 
sentially complementary  since  each  system  emphasizes  certain  characters  at  the  expense  of  others; 
all  the  information  is  present  in  the  primary  records  but  is  not  easily  extracted  from  them  visually. 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN  285 


EYES    SHUT 


ANSWER 


PEAK-TROUGH    PERI06   IfflJICATOR 


FIG.  3.  Alpha  and  theta  activity  in  a  normal  young  subject.  Records  as  described  for  figure  2. 
The  primary  records  are  particularly  complex.  In  the  frequency  analysis  records,  the  solid  lines 
connect  the  peaks  related  to  channel  3,  while  the  dotted  lines  connect  the  peaks  related  to  channel 
5.  This  analysis  reveals  components  in  the  theta  and  alpha  bands  which  fluctuate  independently  in 
the  two  hemispheres.  During  the  first  half  of  the  record  the  subject  was  at  rest  and  the  second 
half  was  replying  to  an  annoying  question.  During  this  phase  the  theta  content  increased  in  the 
transverse  derivation  (channel  5)  after  a  period  of  activation  and  fluctuation  in  skin  resistance 
(G.S.R.).  The  period  analysis  shows  fluctuations  in  wave  intervals  between  about  100  and  140  msec, 
corresponding  to  periods  of  relative  alpha  and  theta  activity.  The  prolonged  theta  activity  is  a  char- 
acteristic of  this  response  to  annoyance. 


EYES    OPEN 


/vv 


;v\^^'J^^A/^M\KJ^\r^\^^f'''^'^     'jhu^f^^^^njvJf'S 


^/V^.^^'^VVM^/;'V^•v»v  "^V'^'"  ^^?,  «\^a^>  ,;•  '^/r 


.^^j\^.r'^/^^^-^^'^-hJ\Jfw-^^  ^"^MlivJ^ 


'\\/4'.^/H^'^\^{f\^ 


\m^ 


FIG.  4.  Delta  and  theta  activity  recorded  from  a  normal  child  aged  3.  Six  primary  records  (above) 
and  frequency  analysis  record  (below).  In  early  youth  these  rhythms  are  characteristically  difTusc 
and  complex;  the  frequency  analysis  shows  almost  equal  abundance  in  the  range  from  i  .5  to  9  cycles 
per  sec.  with  a  peak  in  the  theta  band  at  5  cycles  per  sec.  These  rhythms  are  almost  unaffected  by 
stimulation. 


286 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


PROPERTIES    OF    ALPHA    ACTIVITY    AS    TYPICAL 
OF    INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS 

Early  Rijmrls 

The  early  studies  of  alpha  rhythms  in  human  beings 
were  made  with  straiehtforward  recording  devices — 
mirror  oscillographs,  cathode  ray  oscillographs  and, 
later,  ink  writing  recorders.  These  provide  simple 
graphs  of  voltage  changes  with  respect  to  time;  it  is 
unfortunate  that  the  human  eye  is  severely  limited  in 
its  capacity  to  analyze  a  curve  of  this  sort,  being  at- 
tracted to  the  most  prominent  features  and  tending  to 
ignore  or  misrepresent  the  minor  ones.  Berger's  early 
studies,  original  and  detailed  though  they  were,  pro- 
vided only  limited  information  about  the  distribu- 
tion and  complexity  of  the  alpha  rhythms  because  he 
employed  only  one  or  two  separate  recording  chan- 


nels; his  impression  that  the  alpha  rhythms  were  de- 
veloped by  the  whole  brain  was  due  to  this  limitation. 
During  the  last  20  years  however,  the  technical  trend 
has  been  toward  multiplication  of  channels  and 
elaijoration  of  analyzing  de\ices.  There  is  no  doubt 
that  in  consequence  the  picture  of  alpha  activity  has 
become  progressively  more  and  more  involved  and 
controversial.  Adrian  &  Matthews  (i),  and  Adrian 
&  Yamagiwa  (2)  were  the  first  to  prove  that  the 
alpha  rhythms  usually  arose  in  the  posterior  regions  of 
the  l)rain,  and  they  were  able  to  demonstrate  in  some 
subjects  an  abrupt  change  in  the  sign  of  the  potential 
gradient  of  the  alpha  waves  over  the  scalp,  an  effect 
which  has  since  become  known  as  a  '  phase-re\ersal 
focus'.  In  the  simplest  case,  a  potential  distribution 
of  this  sort  could  be  produced  Ijy  an  equivalent  gen- 
erator within  the  head  oriented  radially  with  its 
axis  projecting  toward  the  surface  at  the  'focus',  and 


T- 


■■^iAfc*iiii»<p*Mi  ijiMy*^!**  I  *^m^i»^ 


, -'^  .^^  n»^ii^w^^>i 


EYES    SHUT     ® 


^--  Nn^MAr'  •NiVk*^*^' 


>^»»■*'»^|«*^\/^^<»»V^ll^^i>^MlM*»^il■■■^«*W■'VN^^^WP^V^yl^j^^^»|»H*W*'^^W*^  /^v  *"v-.,'Vv*'V***'**'\. 


I  Ui  mill  ,1.11 


EYES     OPEN*  EYES    SHUT® 


■MiMM 


'   1,  V 


>      W  V  i- . — .      V  V 


'  ii|inw»iiii-»'[.r||    i'i»<M<iM*»«'>**l''V'V»if»>/«N»»<^V*»«»»«l>,f~»l>»'^>««««/*V<'>«^,ni-l<tNlii')» 
.l»«»**>ii|^»  »    ■  l■■.^~.v^^l><|>■^Wli^|lwl\t<l«W>'|,^lll<Mr^■«^»^MlV^»««^W«*»»»v»<^ir'»«»^'|||■■<»»lj^^f»|«^<^''l|» 


FIG.  5.  Primary  records,  frequency  analysis  and  period  display  from  a  subject  of  the  alpha  M 
type.  Upper  portion  contains  foiu'  primary  records  and  a  frequency  analysis  which  is  continued  in 
the  middle  portion;  the  lowermost  record  is  the  period  display.  Immediately  on  eye  closure  there 
is  a  brief  burst  of  alpha  activity  at  i  o  to  1 1  cycles  per  sec,  but  even  this  is  sometimes  lacking.  The 
spectrum  of  brain  rhythms  is  almost 'white,'  although  the  absence  of  regular  rhythms  makes  the 
faster  activity  seem  more  obvious. 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


287 


this   was   the   schema    tentatively   suggested   for   the 
origin  of  the  alpha  rhythms  in  the  human  brain. 

Individuality  of  Alpha  Rliytlirns  and  their  I  'ariation 

During  the  last  10  years  several  experiments  have 
expanded  and  modified  the  methods  introduced  by 
Adrian  &  Matthews  and  have  also  widened  their 
survey  to  the  study  of  a  large  number  of  subjects. 
These  obser\ations  have  shown  clearly  that,  unlike 
most  physiological  phenomena,  the  alpha  rhythms 
must  be  considered  in  relation  to  each  individual  of  a 
given  species  and  not  merely  as  a  specific  or  generic 
character.  In  other  words  the  alpha  rhythm  patterns, 
in  terms  both  of  spatial  distribution,  frequency  and 
relation  to  function,  are  highly  characteristic  of  every 
indi\idual.  The  variation  is  so  wide  that  classification 
of  alpha  type  must  include  a  class  of  normal  person 
in  whom  no  alpha  activity  whatever  is  visible  (fig.  5), 
even  in  those  conditions  which  are  most  favorable  to 
the  appearance  of  these  rhythms  in  other  people.  At 
the  other  extreme  there  are  people  in  whom  alpha 
rhythms  persist  even  in  circumstances  which  are  most 
inclined  in  other  subjects  to  interrupt  or  suppress  this 
activity.  A  distribution  of  this  sort  is  extremely  difficult 
to  reconcile  with  any  simple  theory  of  spontaneous 
activity,  and  any  general  scheme  to  account  for 
these  phenomena  must  include  considerations  of 
mental  and  even  social  character — aspects  of  human 
existence  which  are  in  general  far  removed  from  the 
domain  of  neurophysiology. 

Accepting  the  need  for  considerable  reserve,  it  is 
generally  true  that  the  amplitude  of  spontaneous 
rhythms  in  the  alpha  category  is  inversely  correlated 
with  visual  attention.  Empirically  the  frequency 
range  acceptable  for  alpha  rhythms  is  from  8  to  13 
cycles  per  sec,  and  the  distribution  of  frequency  in  a 
large  population  follows  a  more  or  less  normal  Gaus- 
sian curve,  the  mode  falling  at  about  10  cycles  per 
.sec.  The  tails  of  the  curve  should  extend  to  about  6 
and  15  cycles  per  .sec;  about  one  in  5,000  individuals 
does  in  fact  show  rhythms  at  these  limits  which  com- 
ply with  the  arbitrary  definition  of  alpha  rhythms. 

Complexity  oj  Alpha  Rhythms 

The  appearance  of  alpha  rhythms  in  normal  people 
is  usually  suggestive  of  intrinsic  complexity  and  var- 
ious methods  of  analysis  have  jjeen  applied  to  the 
study  of  this  possibility.  Whatever  method  is  used  the 
great  majority  of  alpha  rhythms  are  compound  in  the 
sense  that  there  are  usually  several  components  within 
the  alpha  band  (65).  The  superposition  of  these  vari- 


ous components  in  a  record  produces  an  appearance 
of  continuous  but  irregular  modulation;  sometimes 
the  amplitudes  and  frequencies  are  so  constant  over  a 
period  of  time  that  a  regular  pattern  of 'beats'  is  pro- 
duced. The  identification  of  the  various  alpha  com- 
ponents can  also  be  accomplished  geometrically  by 
recording  from  electrode  patterns  in  which  derivations 
may  be  made  from  orthogonal  electrode  chains.  With 
this  arrangement  one  component  may  be  found  to  ije 
more  prominent  in  records  from  anteroposterior 
electrodes  and  another  from  traverse  ones.  Further- 
more, the  components  may  be  distinguished  by  their 
functional  activity  or  responsiveness.  For  example,  in 
those  people  with  persistent  alpha  rhythms,  one  com- 
ponent may  continue  when  the  eyes  are  open  while 
another  is  more  prominent  when  the  eyes  are  shut 
during  mental  activity.  The  geometrical  and  func- 
tional separation  of  alpha  components  is  perhaps 
more  convincing  than  their  display  by  instrumental 
analysis,  but  the  three  methods  can  be  employed  to- 
gether to  construct  a  dynamic  picture  of  spontaneous 
activity  in  relation  to  function  and  behavior.  Other 
methods  of  analysis  of  alpha  rhythms  have  been  pro- 
posed by  Sato  (47),  Krakau  (35),  Burch  (15),  Koz- 
hevnikov  (34)  and  Bekkering  (8).  These  have  not  so 
far  been  applied  to  a  very  wide  range  of  subjects  and 
situations  but  their  trials  have  been  promising  (15,  16, 
24,  25,  36,  66). 

In  experiments  designed  to  exploit  the  three  prin- 
cipal methods  of  analysis,  simultaneous  observations 
of  changes  in  the  autonomic  nervous  system  and  the 
behavior  of  the  subject  are  of  considerable  value. 
These  techniques  have  not  yet  been  fully  developed 
so  the  information  is  still  inadequate  to  give  a  clear 
indication  of  alpha  significance  in  terms  of  somatic 
change.  A  further  difficulty  is  that  such  experiments 
involve  some  selection  of  '  suitable'  subjects  and  this 
has  caused  considerable  difficulty  in  comparing  re- 
sults from  diiTerent  laboratories.  For  example,  there  is 
a  natural  inclination  to  choose  for  study  people  with 
large  regular  alpha  rhythms  since  they  seem  likely  to 
provide  records  which  are  easier  to  measure  and  inter- 
pret. Selection  of  people  in  this  group  inevitably 
limits  the  scope  of  investigation,  and  there  is  even 
some  evidence  that  subjects  with  extremely  prominent 
and  persistent  rhythms  may  display  mental  characters 
bordering  on  the  pathological. 

Identification  of  Alpha  Components 

When  allowance  is  made  for  these  limitations,  there 
remain  certain  general  features  which  seem  incon- 
testable.   First,    the    attenuation    and    constriction    of 


288 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


alpha  activity  during  visual  attention  and  mental  vigi- 
lance is  almost  invariable.  When  means  are  available 
for  instrumental  analysis,  a  convenient  form  for  the 
measure  of  alpha  activity  is  'aisundance'.  This  in- 
cludes dimensions  both  of  amplitude  and  of  persist- 
ence. It  is  comparable  with  a  measure  of  energy,  but 
this  term  is  undesirable  because  of  its  precise  connota- 


FiG.  6.  An  a.ssembly  of  cut-outs  showing  Huctuations  in 
abundance  of  alpha  components  over  long  periods  during  the 
performance  of  various  tasks:  (/I)  in  a  subject  of  the  alpha- 
responsive  type  showing  an  immediate  and  sustained  rise  in  all 
alpha  components  on  eye  closing  and  a  lack  of  response  to 
mental  activity  with  the  eyes  shut;  (/?)  with  greatly  increased 
analyser  gain  the  alpha  components  are  barely  perceptible  in 
an  alpha-M  type  subject;  (C)  in  a  responsive  versatile  subject 
the  various  components  fluctuate  over  a  wide  range  during  the 
performance  of  a  psychological  task. 


tion  in  physical  systems.  When  analyzed  in  terms  of 
the  abundance  of  its  various  components,  the  changes 
of  alpha  activity  during  spontaneous  or  induced  varia- 
tions of  behavior  can  be  plotted  as  abundance  with 
respect  to  time  (fig.  6).  Necessarily,  each  component 
is  arbitrarily  identified  by  its  frequency,  usually  as  a 
whole  number  of  cycles  per  second,  but  a  composite 
plot  of  these  arbitrary  components  reveals  the  nature 
and  extent  of  the  complexity  previously  referred  to. 
Often  the  various  components  exhibit  some  degree 
of  independence,  particularly  during  the  performance 
of  an  exacting  task,  and  the  individuality  of  the  spon- 
taneous rhythms  is  considerably  emphasized  by  this 
procedure.  The  classical  responsive  types  of  alpha 
rhythm  rise  and  fall  inversely  according  to  the  in- 
tensity of  concentration  of  the  subject,  but  during  a 
period  of  tranquillity  the  pattern  of  frequency  and 
distribution  may  remain  almost  constant  so  that  even 
short  samples  of  record  are  similar  to  one  another. 

.At  the  other  extreme,  subjects  showing  little  alpha 
activitN  display  an  extremely  varied  pattern,  even 
during  rest.  Those  alpha  components  which  are 
present  in  such  records  fluctuate  in  abundance  from 
moment  to  moment,  so  that  extremely  long  samples 
of  record  must  be  taken  if  the  samples  are  to  resemble 
one  another. 

This  observation  has  .suggested  that  a  useful  measure 
of  alpha  character  would  be  the  length  of  sample 
necessary  for  all  of  a  set  of  such  samples  to  fall  within 
a  specified  range  of  variation.  This  computation  is 
performed  automatically  with  a  wave  analyzer  fitted 
with  an  electronic  averaging  device.  This  measure 
pro\ides  an  estimate  of  the  repertory  of  a  person's 
alpha  activity  and  has  been  found  to  be  related  to 
the  .scope  and  variety  of  interests  in  a  population  of 
normal  young  adults.  The  aspect  of  cerebral  men- 
tality defined  in  this  way  has  been  termed  versa- 
tility (jo).  Detailed  analysis  of  alpha  responses  has 
suggested  that,  in  some  people  at  least,  relative 
abundance  of  the  slower  alpha  components  may  be 
associated  with  rest  or  inactivity  of  the  mechanisms 
of  internal  imagination,  while  abundance  of  the 
faster  components  is  more  closely  linked  with  the  lack 
of  significant  afferent  signals  from  the  receptors. 
Thus  with  the  eyes  closed  and  the  subject  relaxed,  the 
most  abundant  rhythm  may  be  at  9  cycles  per  sec; 
but  when  the  subject  is  given  a  mental  task  to  per- 
form with  the  eyes  shut,  the  frequency  of  the  dominant 
rhythm  may  seem  to  rise  to,  say,  i  i  c\cles  per  sec.  In 
some  cases,  however,  the  apparent  acceleration  is  due 
to  attenuation  of  the  lower  frequency  components 
during  concentration,   leaving  the  higher  frequency 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


289 


ones  unmasked.  In  this  condition  the  siilyect  is  attend- 
ing to  imaginary  or  endogenous  signals  and  is  ignor- 
ing external  ones. 

The  intricate  and  elusive  relations  between  the 
various  brain  rhythms  and  mental  functions  have 
been  explored  also  by  Mundy-Castle  (43)  who  has 
identified  three  types  of  theta  thythm  and  two  of  beta 
rhythm,  as  well  as  various  categories  of  activity  in  the 
alpha  range  of  frequencies.  The  statistical  relations 
between  types  of  brain  rhythm  and  psychological 
character  have  been  analyzed  in  detail  by  Werre  (66) 
who  concludes  that,  although  no  unique  associations 
can  be  established  between  any  single  EEG  variable 
and  any  specific  psychological  parameter,  none  the 
less  certain  electrical  patterns  are  contingent  on  psy- 
chological grouping.  For  example,  alpha  frequency  is 
related  to  the  performance  of  psychotechnical  tests, 
since  subjects  with  low  frequencies  perform  slowly  but 
steadily,  those  with  high  frequencies  fast  and  regu- 
larly and  those  with  complex  alpha  rhythms  er- 
ratically. 

Although  the  alpha-blocking  effect  is  seen  most 
clearly  in  response  to  visual  stimuli,  it  can  also  be  pro- 
duced in  some  subjects  by  nonvisual  stimuli  which  are 
novel  and  startling.  The  effect  of  nonvisual  stimuli 
usually  wears  off  quite  rapidly,  but  if  an  ineffective 
stimulus  is  then  accompanied  or  followed  by  an 
effective  visual  one  the  neutral  stimulus  may  become 
'conditioned'.  Conditioning  of  alpha  blocking  was 
first  studied  intensively  by  Jasper  &  Shagass  (31) 
and  has  recently  been  extensively  employed  in  the 
experimental  analysis  of  learning  by  Gastaut  ti  al. 
(24).  They  attribute  generalized  desynchronization 
of  intrinsic  rhythms  by  a  novel  stimulus  to  activation 
of  the  brain  stem  reticular  system,  and  local  desyn- 
chronization by  a  specific  stimulus  to  activation  of  the 
thalamic  reticular  system. 

Degree  of  Constancy  and  Range  of  I'aiuilnm 
in  Alpha  Frequemy 

Although  the  frequency  and  distribution  of  alpha 
rhythms  in  any  particular  subject  are  characteristic 
and  individual,  the  rate  of  an  alpha  rhythm  can  some- 
times be  shifted  slightly.  The  range  of  normal  varia- 
tion is  limited  to  a  fraction  of  a  cycle  per  second, 
however,  and  such  changes  cannot  be  identified  and 
measured  ea.sily  in  conventional  records.  The  effect  is 
easily  demonstrated  with  a  toposcope  display  system 
(56,  57,  61,  64)  designed  to  emphasize  and  correlate 
rhythmic  activity  in  many  regions.  An  example  taken 
from  a  normal  subject  is  shown  in  figure  7.  The  fre- 


quency of  the  major  alpha  rhythms  is  here  8.80  cycles 
per  sec.  over  a  wide  area  at  rest.  During  mental  ac- 
tivity the  rhythm  disappears  in  all  but  two  derivations 
in  the  right  centroparietal  region  where  the  frequency 
rises  to  9.45  cycles  per  sec.  and  then  gradually  subsides 
to  its  original  frequency  in  about  90  sec.  (fig.  8).  This 
example  illustrates  two  very  important  features  of 
alpha  activity;  first,  the  extreme  constancy  in  fre- 
quency in  tranquil  conditions;  second,  the  degree  of 
independence  of  the  two  hemispheres  and  even  of 
adjacent  regions  during  activity.  As  can  be  seen  in 
figure  g,  one  minute  after  the  start  of  the  experiment 
when  the  right  centroparietal  region  is  showing  alpha 
rhythm  at  9.1  cycles  per  sec,  the  left  temporoparietal 
derivation  has  resumed  alpha  activity  but  at  9.35 
cycles  per  sec.  Nevertheless,  when  the  period  of  at- 
tention is  over,  all  regions  return  precisely  to  their 
original  rate  of  8.80  cycles  per  .sec. 

This  degree  of  constancy  is  by  no  means  unusual 
and  has  been  reported  afso  by  Brazier  &  Casby 
(12)  and  Barlow  &  Brazier  (5)  using  an  entirely 
different  method  of  analysis  and  correlation.  The  pat- 
tern of  frequency  fluctuation,  however,  is  an  individual 
character  and  is  related  to  the  complexity  of  the  rest- 
ing alpha  activity;  if  there  are  .several  rhythms,  the 
apparent  changes  in  frequency  are  usually  abrupt 
and  extensive  and  may  be  in  the  direction  of  accelera- 
tion or  deceleration.  Changes  of  this  type  are  at- 
tributable to  the  substitution  of  one  process  for 
another  rather  than  to  changes  of  rate  in  the  same 
process.  In  general,  a  particular  alpha  rhythm  seems 
to  be  capable  of  aijout  ±0.5  cycles  per  sec.  variation 
within  the  physiological  range;  a  greater  change  can 
be  induced  by  the  administration  of  drugs  but  such 
alterations  are  associated  with  signs  of  intoxication. 
For  example,  ingestion  of  1 00  ml  of  alcohol  in  i  }'2 
hr.  in  one  subject  reduced  the  alpha  frequency  from 
10  to  9  cycles  per  .sec,  but  the  subject  was  seriously 
inebriated  and  relapsed  into  a  prolonged  stupor  40 
min.  later.  Conversely,  activating  drugs  such  as 
amphetamine  or  pipradrol  compounds  in  sufficient 
doses  may  raise  the  frecjuency  of  an  alpha  rhythm 
by  as  much  as  i  cycle  per  sec,  but  this  is  associated 
with  marked  mental  stimulation  and  agitation  (fig. 
10).  The  effects  of  hallucinogenic  drugs  such  as  LSD 
25  are  also  related  to  mental  change;  doses  sufficient 
to  raise  the  apparent  alpha  frequency,  from  e.  g.  10  to 
12  cycles  per  sec,  induce  characteristic  transforma- 
tions in  mood  and  character. 


290 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7.  Toposcope  display  sys- 
tem record  of  the  effect  of  mental 
activity  on  alpha  frequency  and 
distribution  in  a  normal  subject. 
Each  of  the  22  circular  areas  is 
the  face  of  a  cathode  ray  tube 
connected  to  an  electrode  pair 
over  the  brain.  There  are  thus  22 
channels.  In  each  circuit  there 
has  been  introduced  a  spiral 
scanning  time-base  upon  which 
are  projected  the  variations  in 
brilliance  proportional  to  the 
rhythmic  changes  of  voltage  re- 
sulting from  brain  activity.  In 
this  case  the  rotation  speed  of  the 
signals  was  one-third  of  the  fre- 
quency of  the  resting  alpha 
rhythm,  which  was  8.8  cycles  per 
sec.  The  duration  of  each  expo- 
sure was  8  sec.  The  intrinsic 
rhythms  are  seen  as  white 
smudges  in  each  indicator  tube. 
The  first  exposure  (.4)  shows  the 
alpha  distribution  and  frequency 
at  rest;  during  the  next  exposure 
(B)  the  subject  was  asked  to 
begin  a  series  of  mental  tasks; 
the  immediate  effect  of  the  in- 
structions was  to  suppress  the 
alpha  activity  in  all  but  two 
channels  in  the  right  hemisphere 
where  the  frequency  rose  to  9.4 
cycles  per  sec.  The  subsequent 
exposures  were  taken  during 
performance  of  the  task  and  show 
a  gi-adual  return  to  the  resting 
state.  During  this  period  the 
alpha  activity  decelerated  slowly 
and  returned  to  the  left  hemis- 
phere where  it  remained  accel- 
erated for  a  longer  period  than 
on  the  right  side.  The  return  to 
the  original  condition  took  nearly 
too  sec,  but  the  final  frequency 
was  exactly  the  same  as  before 
the  activation.  [From  Walter 
(61).] 


I^^^^^^^H  ""VLr"  ^ 

gt  w 

^K.  .^^^^B^^Ibs 

IDIBI 

■■■■■ 

I'MB 

gftl^gSk 

|j|^^^9 

1  V-^ 

fl^iPi^ 

B^^^l 

^^^gi 

^^^^■^Er  -*'' '  ^Hu^l 

psj^^pi 

Hr^^ 

E^H^ 

^Mm 

I^M 

IhK<^^^^I 

B^cSr*vt*.  ji^^^^H 

^H^^l 

H  -  m^ 

^^HS 

^Kig 

K^^BI 

^^H| 

^Hp^&a|^ 

^^j|^k9H 

^^|Hm 

»:J» 

|^|£^ 

!■■ 

i' 

/i  < 

•  "-- 

<  f  ^^^Bi 

H 

•  -.f 

m. 

z 

J^ 

^  'S^-' 

% 

k™ 

V' 

(^1 

*'im 

>£ 

%I 

iT 

(. 

^w 

^ 

«  - 

INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


291 


^.    FREQUENCY 


70  8  0  90 

TIME     IN      SECONDS 


FIG.  8.  A  plot  of  the  alpha  frequencies  in  the  right  centroparietal  and  left  temporoparietal  regions 
derived  from  the  experiment  represented  in  figure  7.  Independent  fluctuations  of  the  two  hemispheres 
are  far  outside  the  error  of  observation  and  are  characteristic  of  the  subject.  [From  Walter  (61}.] 


c/s 

12  2 

"o<. 

FREQUENCY 

121 

12  0 

n 

R.OCC. 

\\t 

118 

p 

II  7 
116 
115 

, 

116    c/s 

. 

n  4 

- 

113 
112 
III 

- 

I         ' 

- 

- 

no 

■ 

10  9 

10  8 

10  7 
106 

■ 

SUM 

1 

PGR 

1 

t 

ANSWER                 -f-     1 

t                           PC 

PGR    1 

.       t     * 

R 

Q.      A. 

T    r 

PGR 

.        .4-    . 

1        .        1 

1 

3 

20 

30 

40 

50 
TIME 

60 

IN    SE 

70 

CON 

8 
DS 

0 

90 

100 

FIG.  9.  Fluctuations  in  alpha  frequency  of  another  normal  subject  studied  in  the  same  way  as 
in  figure  7,  exhibiting  complex  alpha  analysis.  The  resting  frequency  of  1 1 .6  cycles  per  sec.  is 
replaced  by  others  at  12  and  10.95  cycles  per  sec.  which  alternate  in  dominance,  but  the  original 
rate  is  returned  to  at  the  end  of  the  activation  period.  [From  Walter  (61).] 


292 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY   I 


97 
96 
95 
94 
9  3 

92 
9( 

90 
89 

e  8 

87 
8« 


INST. 

1 

1 1 

MERATRAN 

SUM                                   ANS. 

1 

1 1 

SUM          A.          A. 

8«C/S 

1                 1         .        1                 1.1 

. 1 . 1 . 1 . 1 . 1 . 1 . 1 ^. 

10 


20 


30 


40 


50 


60 


70 


80  90  100  110 

TIME     IN  SECONDS 


120 


130 


140 


FIG.  lo.  Alpha  frequency  changes  in  the  same  subject  as  in  figures  7  and  8  under  the  influence  of 
pipradrol  (Meratran).  The  steady  frequency  has  risen  by  0.4  cycles  per  sec.  and  the  rate  of  change 
during  attention  is  abrupt  and  discontinuous,  but  again  the  frequency  returns  to  precisely  its  orig- 
inal value.  [From  Walter  (61).] 


Effects  of  Activation  and  Stimulation 

The  effects  of  generalized  or  nonspecific  activation 
on  the  spontaneous  alpha  rhythms  are  sHght  but 
characteristic;  of  equal  interest  are  the  relations  be- 
tween responses  to  specific  physiological  stimuli  and 
the  distribution  and  time  relations  of  the  intrinsic 
rhythms.  The  most  dramatic  and  effective  changes 
are  of  course  those  produced  by  visual  stimulation.  In 
one  of  the  early  reports  on  the  effect  of  flicker,  Adrian 
&  Matthews  (i)  described  these  as  'driving  the  alpha 
rhythm',  l:)ut  more  detailed  study  (42,  53)  has  shown 
that,  except  in  certain  circumstances,  responses  to 
photic  stimulation  are  distinct  from  alpha  waves. 
Those  circumstances  in  which  alpha  rhythms  are  in- 
volved in  evoked  responses  are  of  particular  interest 
in  the  study  of  spontaneous  activity  since  the  interac- 
tion between  rhythmic  external  stimuli  and  intrinsic 
brain  rhythms  indicates  a  possible  function  of  the 
latter.  Bishop  (10,  11)  reported  cyclic  changes  in  the 
excitability  of  the  visual  system  of  the  rabbit  and  the 
relation  of  such  changes  to  intrinsic  alpha  rhythms 
has  Ijeen  studied  by  several  other  experimenters, 
Bartley  &  Bishop  (6),  Gastaut  et  al.  (23),  Lindsley 
(40)  and  Lansing  (37)  In  some  animals  there  seems 
to  be  good  evidence  for  periodic  fluctuations  in  the 
excitability  of  the  visual  system,  but  in  human  sub- 
jects the  observations  are  inconclusive  because  of  the 


wide  variation  between  individuals.  The  properties 
of  the  retina  interfere  .seriously  with  attempts  to  meas- 
ure the  time  relations  between  visual  stimulus  times 
and  evoked  responses;  in  effect,  the  retina  acts  as  an 
integrator  for  brief  flashes  of  light  so  that  the  volley 
of  impulses  in  the  visual  pathways  is  a  very  ragged 
one,  spread  over  a  period  of  o\er  50  m.sec.  even  when 
the  stimulus  is  a  flash  of  light  lasting  only  a  few  micro- 
seconds. This  means  that  even  if  the  central  visual 
structures  were  totally  '  blind'  for  half  of  every  alpha 
wave,  they  could  still  receive  signals  that  fell  in  the 
blind  phase  because  the  afferent  volley  would  outlast 
the  critical  period. 

Synchronization  of  Alpha  Rhythms 

There  is  a  further  complication  to  be  considered; 
the  alpha  rhythms  can  only  be  'driven'  over  a  narrow 
range  of  frequencies,  Ijut  this  range  is  enough  to 
allow  them  to  be  synchronized  or  locked  in  phase  by 
afferent  visual  signals.  This  is  demonstrated  clearly  in 
toposcope  records;  this  device  can  be  arranged  to 
deliver  brief  flashes  of  light  in  a  time-sequence  of 
doublets  or  triplets  at  any  chosen  repetition  rate.  In  this 
way  the  pattern  of  true  evoked  responses  can  be  distin- 
guished from  the  superficially  similar  but  functionally 
distinct  pattern  of  synchronized  alpha  rhythms.  When 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


293 


the  repetition  rate  of  the  flash  groups  is  set  at  about 
the  alpha  frequency  or  a  submuhiple  of  this,  many 
subjects  show  no  signs  of  the  stimulus  pattern  but  only 
a  sharp  synchronization  of  the  alpha  rhythm.  This 
effect  can  also  exist  together  with  an  evoked  pattern 
and  the  two  modes  of  response  may  appear  in  adjacent 
regions  which  may  exchange  modes  from  time  to  time. 
The  effects  of  alpha  driving  and  alpha  synchroniza- 
tion can  combine  to  corrupt  an  evoked  response  by 
interpolation  of  the  '  missing'  component  in  a  triplet 
pattern,  or  by  omission  of  one  of  the  responses. 

The  distinction  between  rhythm  synchronization 
and  true  evocation  is  important  in  the  interpretation 
of  the  results  of  such  experiments;  it  is  not  always 
easy  to  achieve  because  the  identity  of  a  cerebral 
process  can  be  inferred  only  indirectly  from  its  elec- 
trical characters.  However,  with  the  toposcopic  dis- 
play .system  the  peculiar  phase  or  time  relations  of 
the  alpha  rhythms  in  different  parts  of  the  brain  can 
be  used  to  supplement  identification  by  frequency, 
distribution  and  responsiveness.  Records  taken  with 
electrodes  on  the  scalp  almost  always  reveal  clear 
differences  in  the  time  of  appearance  of  alpha 
waves  in  various  regions.  In  general,  the  maximum 
potential  change  is  earlier  in  the  anterior  regions 
than  in  the  occiput,  and  besides  this  anteroposterior 
sweep  there  is  evidence  of  an  even  greater  discrepancy 
in  phase  between  the  longitudinal  and  transverse 
derivations  covering  the  same  region.  The  commonest 
appearance  is  for  the  alpha  waves  in  the  transverse 
derivations  to  lead  those  in  the  longitudinal  ones  by 
90°;  for  example,  the  peak  of  the  waves  seen  in  the 
parietotemporal  channel  occurs  at  the  instant  of  zero 
potential  in  the  parieto-occipital  one.  These  phase  re- 
lations are  so  clear  and  consistent  that  they  can  be 
used  as  a  diagnostic  sign  of  alpha  activity;  when  the 
effect  of  a  stimulus  is  merely  to  synchronize  this  ac- 
tivity, the  characteristic  phase  relations  are  usually 
maintained.  On  the  other  hand,  where  there  is  a 
true  evoked  response,  this  shows  the  expected  latency, 
which  varies  slightly  from  region  to  region,  but  the 
phase  relations  are  not  those  of  the  alpha  activity. 

Even  before  these  details  of  alpha  activity  were 
known  the  pos.sibility  had  Ijeen  considered  that  such 
rhythms  represented  a  more  elaborate  process  than  a 
simple  time  cycle  of  excitability.  It  may  be  supposed 
that  the  regular  rise  and  fall  of  threshold  in  the  brain 
resembles  the  ebb  and  flow  of  a  tide  round  the  globe. 
The  time  of  high  tide,  so  to  say,  varying  from  port  to 
port  will  not  merely  control  the  accessibility  of  the 
various  relav  stations  but  will  also  act  as  a  clock. 


transforming  time  into  space  patterns  and  contrari- 
wise. From  this  conjecture  have  been  derived  a  num- 
ber of  variations  on  the  theme  of  scanning,  elaborated 
by  McCulloch  (41),  Wiener  (67),  Walter  (56)  and 
others.  The  nearest  to  conclusive  evidence  of  such  a 
process  is  the  phenomenon  described  by  Walter  (58) 
as  '  abscission' ;  the  elements  of  a  visual  time  pattern 
are  cut  off  and  projected  in  a  spatial  pattern  in  the 
visual  association  regions  of  the  brain.  The  time  rela- 
tions and  distribution  of  this  effect  suggest  that  the 
sweep  of  alpha  waves  through  the  cortex  may  provide 
the  time-space  transformation.  Auxiliary  subjective 
evidence  is  provided  by  the  illusions  of  mottled  mov- 
ing patterns  of  colored  light  seen  when  gazing  at  a 
featureless  flickering  field.  The  illusions  are  powerful 
enough  to  produce  aberrations  of  color  vision  as  indi- 
cated by  the  Ishihara  test  when  viewed  by  a  flickering 
light  (59)  and  they  are  attributed  to  the  same  cause 
as  the  complex  electric  patterns  evoked  by  flicker — 
interaction  between  rhythmic  volleys  of  impulses  in 
the  visual  pathways  with  the  intrinsic  scanning 
rhythms. 

Evidence  from  Intracerebral  Electrodes 

The  spontaneous  brain  rhythms  as  .seen  in  scalp 
records  seem  to  have  a  characteristic  geometry  as  well 
as  a  proper  frequency  and  relation  to  function.  Such 
records  are  open  to  the  obvious  criticism  that  being 
derived  from  electrodes  on  the  surface  of  the  head, 
they  can  represent  only  the  average  field  of  vast  ag- 
gregations of  neural  units,  all  remote  from  the  elec- 
trodes in  terms  of  neuronic  dimensions.  Additional 
information  is  now  a\ailable  from  investigations  with 
microelectrodes  placed  in  or  near  to  individual 
neurons  and  their  processes  (38)  and  also  from  elec- 
trodes implanted  in  the  brains  of  human  subjects  for 
clinical  study  (20,  48,  49). 

These  methods  are  still  in  the  early  stages  of  de- 
velopment, but  they  have  nevertheless  already  indi- 
cated that  even  in  the  intimate  details  of  brain 
mechanisms  spontaneous  rhythmic  activity  is  a  dis- 
tinct phenomenon;  it  cannot  be  considered  as  an 
aggregate  or  envelope  of  unitary  neuronic  spike 
discharges.  Nor  is  there  any  invariable  relation  be- 
tween the  spontaneous  wave-like  potential  changes 
near  a  neinon  and  its  all-or-none  action  potentials. 
When  the  probability  of  a  cortical  unit  discharging  is 
low,  then  its  rate  of  firing  may  be  governed  to  some 
extent  by  the  field  of  the  spontaneous  rhythms;  unit 
spikes  are  seen  more  commonly  in  the  phase  when 


FIG.  I  I .  Records  taken  with  the  toposcope  in  the  laboratory 
of  Dr.  Sem-Jacobsen  in  Oslo  from  a  psychopathic  patient  in 
whom  electrodes  had  been  implanted  intracerebrally  three 
days  earlier.  A.  The  approximate  position  of  the  electrodes  for 
the  22  indicator  tubes,  derived  from  X-ray  projection  drawings. 
The  anterior  channels  were  connected  to  electrodes  deep  in 
the  medial  structures,  the  posterior  ones  were  in  the  middle  of 
the  parietal  and  occipital  lobes.  B.  The  distribution  of  the 
intrinsic  alpha  rhythms  with  the  eyes  shut.  There  are  three 
distinct  components,  one  at  8.8  cycles  per  sec.  in  channel  14, 
another  at  8.5  cycles  per  sec.  in  channels  16  and  18  through 
22,  and  a  third  at  915  cycles  per  sec.  in  channel  17.  There  is 
also  a  theta  rhythm  at  7  cycles  per  sec.  in  channel  5.  The  alpha 
rhythms  in  the  posterior  regions  show  the  characteristic  phase 
differences  suggesting  a  moving  source.  The  signals  in  channel 
8  are  artefacts  arising  at  a  high-resistance  electrode.  C.  Record 
made  a  few  seconds  later  during  flicker  stimulation  at  8.8 
Hickers  per  sec.  The  only  regions  showing  clear  fundamental 
synchronization  arc  those  corresponding  to  channels  i  i  and  1 7 
and  19.  Channel  14  shows  deceleration  to  8.6  cycles  per  sec, 
channel  20  is  unaffected  and  channels  21  and  22  show  a  re- 
sponse at  twice  the  stimulus  rate.  D.  Flicker  .stimulation  with 
triplet  groups  of  flashes  at  4.2  groups  per  sec.  evokes  a  replica  of 
the  stimulus  pattern  only  in  channels  18  and  20.  Channels  14, 


294 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


-'95 


the  region  of  the  neuron  concerned  is  electronegative 
with  respect  to  the  surrounding  tissue.  Li  et  al.  (38) 
have  suggested  that  the  microlocation  of  spontaneous 
rhythms  in  the  layers  II  to  V  of  cat  cortex  may  be 
similar  to  that  of  the  recruiting  responses  evoked  by 
thalamic  stimulation.  Stimulation  of  this  type  is 
effective  only  when  its  frequency  is  close  to  that  of  the 
spontaneous  cortical  rhythms,  that  is  at  5  to  8  cycles 
per  sec,  and  this  relation  is  reflected  in  a  correspond- 
ence between  the  phase  of  the  recruiting  response, 
the  spontaneous  rhythms  and  the  unit  discharges.  It 
is  not  clear,  however,  whether  the  spontaneous  ac- 
tivity in  these  preparations  is  functionally  homologous 
with  the  alpha  rhythms  in  human  subjects. 

In  some  cases,  therefore,  the  spontaneous  rhythms 
can  act  as  electrotonic  escapements,  but  there  are 
many  occasions  when  the  spontaneous  rhythms  and 
unit  discharges  are  quite  unrelated.  This  \ariability  is 
manifest  even  in  conditions  of  normal  adaptation; 
Ricci  et  al.  (46)  have  described  the  complexities  of 
the  relations  between  unit  firing  and  surface  rhythms 
in  the  occipital  cortex  of  monkey  during  the  estab- 
lishment of  conditioned  responses  to  sounds  associated 
with  light  flashes  at  7  per  sec.  They  conclude  that 
such  a  response  "is  a  complex  pattern  of  interwoven 
inhibitory  and  excitatory  processes",  in  which  the 
electric  fields  of  relatively  slow  wave-like  spontaneous 
rhythms  are  interlaced  with  the  rapid  all-or-none 
discharges  of  individual  cells.  A  similar  image  was 
employed  by  \Valter  C58)  to  describe  the  topologic 
details  of  evoked  and  spontaneous  activity  in  human 
subjects  engaged  in  learning:  "an  interweaving  of  re- 
ciprocal electric  filaments  to  generate  an  intricate  and 
duraijle  texture  of  significant  association." 

Such  observations  suggest  that  an  important  factor 
in  cerebral  mechanisms  must  be  the  geometry  of  the 
electric  fields  in  the  region  of  neurons  and  their 
processes.  It  is  often  forgotten  that  these  fields  have 
vectorial  as  well  as  scalar  aspects — they  have  direc- 
tion as  well  as  magnitude.  As  already  mentioned,  the 
geometric  and  time  relations  of  alpha  rhythms  as 
seen  on  the  scalp  might  be  due  to  the  adventitious 
effect  of  remoteness  from  the  source  in  a  volume  con- 
ductor. Walter  &  Dovey  (63)  reported  observations 
of  alpha  rhythms  in  the  depths  of  the  occipital  lobe  in 


patients  investigated  for  the  delimitation  of  cerebral 
tumors,  but  they  could  not  study  the  details  of  this 
activity.  Recently  Cooper  et  al.  (18)  have  been  able 
to  obtain  toposcopic  records  of  alpha  rhythms  derived 
from  electrodes  implanted  in  the  brains  of  patients 
with  no  organic  brain  disease.  In  these  experiments 
the  subjects  were  provided  with  up  to  70  fine  wire 
electrodes  in  various  regions  of  the  brain  as  described 
by  Dodge  et  al.  (20)  and  Sem-Jacobsen  et  al.  (48).  In 
one  subject  it  was  possible  to  record  from  intracerebral 
electrodes  connected  to  the  amplifiers  in  the  network 
pattern  customary  for  toposcope  studies  of  scalp  po- 
tentials. These  records  (fig.  1 1)  of  alpha  activity  from 
the  depths  of  the  brain  show  phase — and  space — re- 
lations quite  similar  to  those  found  in  the  superficial 
fields.  The  efTects  of  synchronization  by  photic  stimu- 
lation and  of  blocking  by  attention  were  also  similar. 
As  reported  by  Sem-Jacobsen  et  al.  C49),  the  greatest 
amplitude  of  the  alpha  rhythms  was  found  about  2 
cm  below  the  surface  of  the  brain,  but  the  rhythm 
existed  also  between  pairs  of  electrodes  4  to  5  cm 
deeper.  This  extension  of  the  alpha  activity  could  not 
be  due  to  purely  electric  conduction,  since  the  phase 
of  the  waves  was  not  alwaxs  identical  in  the  various 
regions  and  bursts  of  activity  sometimes  occurred  in 
one  region  and  not  in  others. 

The  conclusion  from  these  studies  is  that  some 
alpha  rhythms  involve  deep  structures  as  well  as  cor- 
te.x  and  the  time  relations  of  the  alpha  waves  indicate 
.some  sort  of  spread  from  front  to  back  and  depth  to 
surface.  The  tendency  of  the  transverse  components 
of  the  rhythm  to  be  phase-shifted  by  90°  with  respect 
to  the  longitudinal  ones  suggests  that  there  may  be  two 
interlocked  processes,  one  generated  by  a  corticobasal 
mechanism,  the  other,  essentially  corticocortical  with 
peaks  corresponding  in  phase  to  the  moment  of  most 
rapid  potential  change — that  is,  zero  potential — of 
the  corticobasal  process. 

Relation  Between  .Alfiha  Rhythms  and  Effector  Fiuution 

The  possibility  that  the  alpha  cycle  may  act  as  a 
gating  mechanism  for  afferent  signals  has  suggested 
that  a  similar  relationship  might  be  found  for  efferent 
ones.  Kibbler  et  al.  (32),  Kibbler  &  Richter  (33)  and 


16  and  19  are  synchronized  at  twice  the  group  rate  (8.4  cycles 
per  sec.)  and  channels  21  and  22  respond  at  4  X  42  =  16.8 
cycles  per  sec.  E.  Flicker  stimulation  with  triplets  at  5.6  groups 
per  sec.  evokes  true  replica  in  channel  1 7,  rhythms  at  1 1 .2  cycles 
per  sec.  in  channels  16,  18,  19  and  20,  and  at  3  X  5.6  =  16.8 
in  channels  21  and  22.  All  these  patterns  were  associated  con- 


sistently with  specific  frequencies  and  modes  of  stimulation 
and  were  similar  to  those  deri\'ed  from  scalp  records.  The 
arithmetic  and  geometric  relations  of  the  various  features  in 
such  records  suggest  the  presence  of  a  number  of  mechanisms, 
each  with  its  own  domain,  intrinsic  rhythmicity  and  responsive- 
ness to  stimulation. 


296 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Bates  (7)  reported  a  tendency  for  the  \oluntary  move- 
ments of  human  sulsjects  to  be  synchronized  in  phase 
with  their  alpha  rhythms,  and  this  effect  has  been 
observed  in  several  ways.  The  experiments  of  Lansing 
(37)  indicate  that  the  shortest  and  longest  visuomotor 
reaction  times  in  some  human  subjects  tend  to  fall  at 
points  about  50  msec,  apart  in  opposite  phases  of  the 
alpha  cycle.  There  was  also  some  relation  between 
motor  response  and  spontaneous  tremor,  a  fact  sug- 
gesting that  cyclic  changes  in  excitability  were  operat- 
ing also  at  the  level  of  the  spinal  motoneuron  pool. 
This  is  perhaps  to  be  expected  since  the  control  of 
voluntary  movement  is  not  belie\ed  to  depend  upon 
complex  activation  of  reflex  circuits  through  high 
central  structures.  Obser\ations  of  these  relations 
have  so  far  yielded  only  statistical  information;  there 
is  no  clear  indication  of  how  the  timing  and  gating 
mechanism  operates,  and  there  are  many  exceptions 
to  any  rule  that  can  be  formulated.  Experiments  with 
auditory  rather  than  visual  stimuli  have  given  even 
less  conclusive  results  (44).  In  planning  and  inter- 
preting such  experiments  it  is  important  to  allow  for 
the  tendency  of  some  intrinsic  rhythms  to  be  pulled  in 
to  synchrony  by  the  signals  which  they  or  other 
rhythms  may  in  turn  control.  This  interaction,  to- 
gether with  integration  b\-  receptors,  may  be  responsi- 
ble for  the  discrepancies  in  the  reports  by  different 
observers. 


EJfect  of  Temperature  Changes 

The  ease  with  which  alpha  frequency  can  be  meas- 
ured and  its  constancy  in  normal  conditions  has  en- 
couraged the  study  of  the  effects  of  metabolic  change. 
Hoagland  (28)  induced  fever  artificially  and  reported 
a  rise  in  alpha  frequency  with  temperature  of  nearly 
0.5  cycles  per  sec.  per  degree  C  in  normal  subjects 
and  an  even  higher  temperature  coefficient  in 
syphilitic  patients.  Krakau  (36),  using  an  optical 
method  of  frequency  analysis,  was  unable  to  confirm 
this  effect  in  all  his  subjects  and  suggested  that  what- 
ever changes  may  occur  during  artificial  fever  might 
be  due  to  the  general  arousal  by  the  situation  as  much 
as  to  the  rise  in  temperature.  This  seems  likely,  since 
measurement  of  alpha  frequency  in  a  few  subjects 
with  the  toposcope  has  shown  no  regular  variation  of 
alpha  frequency  with  the  normal  diurnal  changes  of 
body  temperature  which  would  be  expected  to  result 
in  fluctuations  of  about  0.25  cycles  per  sec.  It  would 
seem  that  the  alpha  mechanisms  are  to  some  extent 
protected  from  the  primary  efTects  of  temperature 
change. 


DELT.'K,    THETA    AND    BETA    RHYTHMS 

Relation  of  Delta  and  Tlieta  Rhythms  to  Age 

As  a  representative  of  the  class  of  intrinsic  rhythms, 
alpha  activity  is  unique  in  its  close  and  clear  relation 
to  sensory  function.  The  other  forms  of  rhythmic  ac- 
tivity in  the  human  brain,  designated  by  Greek 
letters  for  convenience  rather  than  clarity,  are  more 
familiar  in  clinical  than  in  physiological  studies,  but, 
apart  from  the  paroxysmal  discharges  associated 
uniquely  with  epilepsy,  all  are  found  in  normal  condi- 
tions. Delta  and  thcta  rhythms  are  characteristic  of 
infancy  and  childhood,  accompanying  the  maturation 
of  normal  children  in  a  highly  variable  but  significant 
manner  (60).  They  precede,  but  in  later  years  are 
often  mingled  with,  adult  alpha  rhythms  and  their 
rate  of  subsidence  is  a  common  measure  of  develop- 
ment. 

Delta    Rhythms 

When  delta  rhythms  persist  appreciably  beyond 
the  age  of  10  or  12,  the  character  of  the  child 
is  usually  suggestive  of  immaturity  in  a  particular 
fashion  to  which  Hodge  et  al.  (29)  have  given 
the  name  ductility,  the  tendency  to  be  led  easily.  In 
the  extreme  this  is  associated  with  minor  recidivist 
delinquency  combined  with  an  appealing  personality. 
In  such  cases  the  delta  rhythms  are  often  most  prom- 
inent in  the  right  teinporo-occipital  region  and  show 
little  responsiveness  to  stimulation.  Another  type  of 
delta  rhythm  is  seen  in  some  children  with  immature 
but  not  necessarily  defective  personalities.  This  is  bi- 
lateral, monorhythmic  and  strikingly  responsi\e  to 
stimulation,  acting  almost  as  an  alpha  rhythm,  par- 
ticularly when  it  is  localized  to  the  occipital  lobes.  In 
otherwise  normal  people  there  is  no  obvious  explana- 
tion of  this  effect,  but  a  similarly  responsive  rhythmic 
slow  activity  is  seen  also  in  patients  with  organic  dis- 
turbance of  deep  midline  structures,  and  it  is  pos- 
sible, therefore,  that  this  type  of  slow  responsive 
rhvthm  is  an  expression  of  inadequacy  of  the  diffuse 
arousal  systems.  Corroboration  of  this  can  sometimes 
be  obtained  from  suppression  of  the  delta  rhythms  by 
administration  of  activating  drugs,  such  as  ampheta- 
mine, which  are  believed  to  act  on  the  diffuse  ascend- 
ing reticular  formation. 

The  association  of  delta  activity  with  disease, 
dystrophy,  damage  and  deep  sleep — from  which 
alliteration  the  phenomenon  was  accorded  its  designa- 
tion— has  suggested  that  it  may  have  some  sort  of 
limiting  or  protective  function  (54,  -,6\  This  notion 


INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS    OF    THE    BRAIN 


297 


has  been  called  by  \^'alter  the  phylactic  hypothesis. 
Since  the  brain  is  poorly  endowed  with  certain  of 
the  protective  devices  conducive  to  the  preservation 
of  other  organs — such  as  pain  and  repair — it  is  reason- 
able to  conjecture  that  some  mechanism  may  exist  to 
constrain  or  restrict  the  influence  of  conditions  likely 
to  initiate  excessive  and  persistent  actixity.  The  rela- 
tively great  size  and  wide  extent  of  delta  rhythms — 
which  may  reach  potential  differences  of  i  mv  on 
the  scalp — suggest  that  as  electrotonic  inhibitors  they 
may  in  times  of  distress  give  the  brain  a  chance  to 
survive  through  inactivation  of  its  cells. 

Theta  Rhythms 

The  class  of  theta  rhythms — which  were  at  first  con- 
fused with  slow  alpha  rhythms  and  later  identified  as 
related  to  thalamic  lesions  (62) — is  most  character- 
istically associated  in  normal  young  people  with  feel- 
ings of  disappointment  and  frustration.  They  are 
evoked  most  easily  by  the  termination  or  withdrawal 
of  an  authentic  agreeable  stimulus  and  often  show  a 
markedly  stereotyped  pattern  of  growth  and  decline 
over  a  period  of  about  20  sec.  or  so  following  such  an 
experience.  Clinically  their  persistence  is  linked  with 
psychopathic  character  traits.  It  has  been  suggested 
(55)  that,  if  the  alpha  rhythms  be  considered  as  scan- 
ning for  visual  pattern,  then  theta  rhythms  may  rep- 
resent a  scanning  for  visceral  pleasure.  Such  an 
analogy  of  analogies  is  notoriously  meretricious,  but 
if  comprehension  is  to  grow,  some  working  hypothesis 
must  be  formulated,  and  at  least  experiments  can  be 
planned  to  discover  why,  just  as  the  alpha  rhythms 
wax  great  at  the  moment  when  patterns  are  excluded 
by  closing  the  eyes,  so  theta  rhythms  tend  to  arise  at 
the  conclusion  of  pleasure. 

Beta  Rhythms 

The  beta  rhythms,  which  were  the  second  class  of 
brain  activity  to  be  identified  by  Berger,  are  still  un- 
certain in  their  significance  and  even  in  their  defini- 
tion. Mundy-Castle  (43)  has  proposed  that  beta 
rhythms  be  considered  in  two  classes,  beta  I  and  beta 
II.  Beta  I  is  suppressed  during  cortical  activity  and  is 
often,  though  not  invariably,  harmonically  related  to  a 
component  of  the  alpha  rhythms;  this  relationship  is 


responsible  for  the  wave  form  of  the  rythme  en  arceau  of 
Gastaut  (22).  In  order  to  avoid  further  confusion  in 
this  already  disordered  domain,  it  might  be  conven- 
ient to  designate  his  particular  combination  of  rhythms 
the  ■  ^'  rhythm  because  of  its  resemblance  to  the  outline 
of  the  Greek  letter.  The  beta  II  of  Mundy-Castle  is  aug- 
mented during  cortical  activity  and  may  represent 
an  acceleration  or  concentration  of  efferent  activity 
arising  from  the  .scansion  of  cortical  regions  engaged 
in  the  analysis  of  endogenous  or  exogenous  patterns. 
Precise  classification  of  these  rhythms — which  are 
particularly  elusive  because  of  their  rate  and  the 
restriction  of  their  domains — must  await  further  study 
of  their  location  and  functional  correlates. 


ORIGIN    OF    INTRINSIC    RHYTHMS 

The  various  intrinsic,  apparently  spontaneous,  yet 
often  responsive,  electrical  rhythms  of  the  brain  are 
clearly  in  a  different  class  of  phenomenon  from  the 
unitary  propagated  spike  potentials  which  act  as  the 
main  operational  code  elements  in  the  nervous  system. 
The  slower  rhythmic  oscillations  seem  more  likely  to 
be  involved,  as  it  were,  in  the  administrative  depart- 
ments of  central  neurophysiology.  If  their  rate  of  dis- 
charge were  less  constant  or  their  wave  form  less  pure, 
they  might  be  considered  as  trivial  projections  of 
spatially  asymmetric  postsynaptic  potentials  in  large 
populations  of  pyramidal  cells  with  particularly  long 
dendritic  proces.ses.  It  is  indeed  conceivable  that  the 
degree  of  asymmetry  and  the  electric  moment  of  the 
dendritic  potentials  might  be  large  enough  in  some 
circumstances  to  generate  the  fields  observed  on  the 
scalp.  Even  if  this  is  the  mechanism  of  generation, 
however,  the  gross  variations  from  person  to  person 
and  the  delicate  relations  between  the  frequencies, 
time  relations  and  geometric  properties  of  the  rhythms 
with  the  character  and  actions  of  the  organism  suggest 
that  the  intrinsic  rhythms  are  more  than  the  resultant 
of  adventitious  topography.  The  processes  of  evolu- 
tion are  too  parsimonious  to  allow  such  entities  to  be 
multiplied  beyond  necessity.  The  refineinents  of 
modern  techniques  should  enable  schools  of  investi- 
gators trained  in  complementary  disciplines  to  solve 
this  enigma  which  so  impedes  our  understanding  of 
brain  function. 


REFERENCES 


1.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  B.  H.  C.  Matthews.  Brain57:  355, 1934. 

2.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  K.  Yamagiwa.  Brain  58:  323,  1935. 


3.  Arvanitaki,  a.  Arch,  internal,  pkjsiol.  52;  381,  1942. 

4.  ."^SHBY,  VV.  R.  Design  for  a  Brain.  London:  Chapman,  1952. 


298 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH\SIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


5.  Barlow,  J.  S.  and  M.    A.    B.    Brazier.    FJeclroencephalog. 
&  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  6:  231,  1954. 

6.  Bartlf.y,  S.   H.  and  G.    H.   Bishop.   Am.  J.   Physiol.   103: 

■  73.  igaa- 

7.  Bates,    J.   A.   V.   Eleclrormephaliio.  G?   CUn.  .Keurophyswl.   2: 
227,  1950. 

8.  Bekkering,   D.    H.    El>r/ioi'riiephrilii<i.  &    Clin.   jXetirophysiol. 
8:  721,  1956. 

9.  Beroer,  H.  Arch.  Psychiat.  87:  527,  1929. 

10.  Bishop,  G.  H.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  103:  213,  1933. 

1 1.  Bishop,  G.  H.  Arch.  Ophth.,  Chicago  14:  992,  1935. 

12.  Brazier,  M.  A.  B.  and  J.  U.  Casbv.  Eleclroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  .\'europhysiol.  4:  210,  1952. 

13.  Bremer,   F.   Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .\eun>phy.uol.    1:   177, 

1949- 

14.  Bullock,   T.   H.    and   C.    .\.   Terzuolo.   J.   Physiol.    138: 

341.  1957- 

15.  BuRCH,  N.  R.,  T.  H.  Greiner  and  E.  G.  Correll.  Fed. 
Proc.  14:23,  1955. 

16.  BuRCH,  N.  R.,  A.  J.  Silverman  and  'I".  H.  Greiner. 
Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  8:  157,   1956. 

17.  Burns,  B.  D.  J.  Physiol.  1 12:  156,  1951. 

18.  Cooper,  R.,  G.  0hrn,  C.  Patten,  C.  W.  Sem-Jacobsen 
and  W.  G.  Walter.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Veurophysiol. 
In  press. 

19.  Cowan,  S.  L.  AND  VV.  G.  Walter.  J.  P/;),wo/.  91  :  loi,  1937. 

20.  Dodge,  H.  W.,  A.  .\.  Bailev,  C.  B.  Holman,  M.  C.  Peter- 
sen AND  C.  W.  Sem-Jacobsen.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  6:  599,  1954. 

2 1 .  Fessard,  a.   In :  Brain  Mechanisms  and  Consciousness,  edited 
by    E.    D.    Adrian,    F.    Bremer,   H.    H.    Jasper   and  J.    F. 
Delafresnaye.  0.\ford :  Blackwell,  1 954. 
Gastaut,  H.  Rev.  neurol.  87:  176,  1952. 
Gastaut,  H.,  Y.   Gastaut,  A.   Roger,  J.   Corriol  and 
R.   Naquet.   Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  3:  401, 

■95'- 

Gastaut,  H.,  A.  C.  Jus,  F.  Morrel,  W.  Storm  van 
Leeuwen,  S.  Doniger,  R.  Naquet,  H.  Regis,  A.  Roger, 
D.  Bekkering,  A.  Kamp  and  J.  Werre.  Eleclroencephalog. 
&  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  9:1,  1957. 

25.  Gershuni,  G.  and  V.  A.  Kozhevnikov.  Problems  of  Ihe 
Modern  Physiology  oj  the  .Nervous  System.  Tiflis:  Publ.  House, 
Acad,  of  Sciences,  Georgian  SSR,  1 956,  p.  53. 

26.  Hagiwara,  S.  .\nd  T.   H.   Bullock.   Biol.  Bull.    109:  341, 

1955- 

27.  Harris,  J.  E.  and  H.  P.  Whiting.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  31:  501, 

1954- 

28.  Hoagland,  H.  .im.  J.  Physiol.  116:  604,  1936. 

29.  Hodge,  R.  S.,  V.  J.  Walter  and  W.  G.  Walter.  Brit.  J. 
Delinq.  3.1,  1953. 

30.  HoDGKiN,  A.  L.  J.  Physiol.  90;  183,  1937. 

31.  Jasper,  H.  H.  and  C.  Shag  ass.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  28:  373, 

1941. 

32.  Kibbler,  G.  O.,  J.  L.  Boreham  and  D.  Riciiter.  .Nature, 
London  164:  371,  1949. 

33.  Kibbler,  G.  O.  and  D.  Richier.  Eleclroencephalog.  cj?  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  2:  227,   1950. 

34.  Kozhevnikov,  V.  A.  Physiol.  J.,  I'SSR  43:  983,  1957. 

35.  Krakau,  C.  E.  T.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin,  .\europhysiol. 
3:97.  195'- 


22. 
23- 


24- 


36.  Krakau,  C.  E.  T.  and  G.  E.  Nv.m.^n.  Ada  physiol.  scandmav. 
29:  281,  1953. 

37.  Lansing,   R.   W.   Eleclroencephalog.  &   Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  9: 

497.  '957- 

38.  Li,  C.  L.,  C.  Cullen  and  H.  H.  Jasper.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  19: 

"31.  1956. 

39.  LiLLiE,  R.  S.  Protoplasmic  Action  and  .Nervous  Action.  Chicago: 
LTniv.  Chicago  Press,  1923. 

40.  Lindsle^',    D.    B.    Eleclroencephalog.  &   Clin.   .Keurophysiol.   4: 

443.  '952- 

41.  McCuLLOCH,  W.  S.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol. 
Suppl.  2,  1949. 

42.  MuND'i'-CASTLE,  A.  C.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neuro- 
physiol. 5:  187,  1953. 

43.  Mundv-Castle,  a.  C.  Eleclroencephalog.  <S  Clm.  .Neuro- 
physiol. 9:643,  1957. 

44.  O'Hare,  J.  J.  Factorial  .Study  of  EEC  and  Auditory  Functions. 
Washington:  Catholic  Univ.  .\m.,  1957. 

45.  Paton,  S.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  21  :  345,  1911. 

46.  Ricci,  G.,  B.  Doane  and  H.  H.  Jasper.  IV  Congr.  Internal. 
Electro-encephalog.  el  .Neurophysiol.,  Rapp.,  Discus,  el  Document. 
Acta  med.  belg.,  1957,  p.  401. 

47.  Sato,  K.  Folia  psychiat.  neurol.,  Japan  5:  198,  1952. 

48.  Sem-Jacobsen,  C.  W.,  R.  G.  Bickford,  M.  C.  Petersen 
and  H.  W.   Dodge.   Proc.  Slajf  Meet.,  Mayo  Clin.  28:   156, 

1953- 

49.  Sem-Jacobsen,  C.  W.,  M.  C.  Petersen,  H.  W.  Dodge, 
J.  A.  Lazarte  and  C.  Holm.ilN.  Eleclroencephalog.  cif  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  8;  263,  1956. 

50.  Shipton,  J.  and  W.  G.  Walter.  In;  Conditionnment  el 
Reaclivite  en  Electroencephalographie,  edited  by  H.  Fischgold 
and  H.  Gastaut.  Paris:  Masson,  1957,  p.   185. 

51.  Tauc,  L.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  47:  769,  1955. 

52.  LIttley,   a.    M.    Eleclroencephalog.  &    Clm.   .Neurophysiol.   6: 

479.  1954- 

53.  Walter,  V.  J.  and  W.  G.  W'alter.  Eleclroencephalog.  &.  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.   i  ;  57,   1949. 

54.  Walter,  W.  G.  J.  .Neurol.  .Neurosurg.& Psychiat.  i  :  359,  1938. 

55.  Walter,  W.  G.  J.  Ment.  Sc.  96:  i,  1950. 

56.  Walter,   W.   G.    The  Living  Brain.   London:   Duckworth. 

1953- 

57.  Walter,    W.    G.    Eleclroencephalog.   &    Clin.    .Neurophysiol. 

Suppl.  4:  7,  1953. 

58.  Walter,  W.  G.  In:  Brain  .Mechanisms  and  Consciousness, 
edited  by  E.  D.  .Xdrian,  F.  Bremer,  H.  H.  Jasper  and  J.  F. 
Delafresnaye.  0.\ford :  Blackwell,  1 954. 

59.  Walter,  W.  G.  Nature,  London  177:  710,  1956. 

60.  Walter,  W.  G.  In:  Child  Development,  edited  by  J.  M. 
Tanner  and  B.  Inhelder.  London:  Tavistock  Press,   1956. 

61.  Walter,  W.  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  50:  799,  1957. 

62.  Walter,  W.  G.  and  V.  J.  Dovey.  J.  Neurol.  .Neurosurg.  & 
Psychiat.  7:57,  1944. 

63.  Walter,  W.  G.  and  V.  J.  Dovey.  Lancet  2:  5,  1946. 

64.  Walter,  W.  G.  and  H.  W.  Shipton.  Brit.  Inst.  Radio 
Engin.  2:  260,  1 95 1. 

65.  Walter,  W.  G.  and  J.  Shipton.  In:  Conditionnment  el 
Reactivite  en  Electroencephalographie,  edited  by  H.  Fischgold 
and  H.  Gastaut.  Paris:  Masson,  1957,  p.   177. 

66.  Werre,  P.  F.  Relations  between  EEC  and  Psychological  Data  in 
.Normal  Adults.  Leiden:  Leiden  Univ.  Press,  1957. 

67.  Wiener,  N.  Cybernetics.  New  York:  Wiley,  1948. 

68.  Wintrebert,  P.  Arch.  Zool-  Exper.  Gen.  60:  221,  1920. 


CHAPTER    XII 


The  evoked  potentials 


HSIANG-TUNG   CHANGE 


Rockefeller  Institute,  New  York  City,  and  the  Institute  of  Neurophysiology, 
University  0/  Copenhagen,  Denmark 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Introduction 

Definition 

Limitations  of  Evoked   Potentials  as  Tools   for  Anatomical 
Study- 
Components  of  Evoked  Potentials  and  Their  Identification 

The  Latent  Period 

Effect  of  Repetitive  Stimulation 

Effect  of  Changes  in  Internal  and  External  Milieu 

Anatomical  Considerations 
Neural  Mechanisms   for   the   Elaboration  of  Evoked   Cortical 
Potentials 

Types   of  Neural    Elements    Involved    and   Their   Mode   of 
Action 

The     Initiation    of    Postsynaptic     Impulses     in     Pyramidal 
Neurons 

Apical  Dendrites  and  Electrical  Signs  of  the  Evoked  Poten- 
tial 

Microelectrode  Findings  Concerning  the  Mechanism  of  Im- 
pulse Initiation  in  Single  Neurons 
After-Discharges 

Repetitive  Firing  of  Individual  Neurons 

Local  After-Discharges  Involving  Intrinsic  Neuronal  Circuits 

Rhythmic     After-Discharges      Involving      Long     Neuronal 
Circuits:  Corticothalamic  Reverberatory  Activity 
Excitability  Changes  Accompanying  and  Following  the  Evoked 
Potential 

Refractory  Periods 

Postexcitatory  Depression 

Periodic  Variation  in  Cortical  Excitability 

Interaction  of  Afferent  Impulses  in  the  Cerebral  Cortex 

Modification  of  Cortical  Excitability  by  Constant  Inflow  of 
Afferent  Impulses 
Summary 


INTRODUCTION 

Definition 

BY  AN  EVOKED  POTENTIAL  IS  meant   the  detectable 
electrical  change  of  any  part  of  the  brain  in  response  to 

'  Present  address:  Academia  Sinica,  Shanghai,  China. 


deliberate  stimulation  of  a  peripheral  sense  organ,  a 
sensory  nerve,  a  point  on  the  sensory  pathway  or  any 
related  structure  of  the  sensory  system.  Although 
observations  of  evoked  potentials  have  most  fre- 
quently been  made  in  the  sensory  system,  potentials 
produced  by  other  means  such  as  by  direct  electrical 
stimulation  or  antidromic  stimulation  of  the  neuron 
fall  into  the  same  category. 

In  physiology  the  term  'potential'  is  often  very 
loosely  used  as  signifying  merely  the  electrical  change. 
Strictly  speaking,  the  potential  at  a  point  of  the  tissue 
implies  the  same  meaning  as  in  physics  and  thus 
denotes  the  work  necessary  to  bring  a  unit  charge  from 
infinity  up  to  the  point  in  question  which  is  located  in 
an  electrostatic  field.  Its  absolute  value  can  be 
measured  only  with  one  electrode  placed  on  the  active 
tissue  and  the  other  grounded.  It  would  be  necessary 
to  record  the  current  rather  than  the  potential  if  the 
two  recording  electrodes  were  placed  on  the  same 
active  tissue.  However,  it  is  possible  to  obtain  a  close 
approximation  of  the  potential  value  by  means  of 
Laplacian  placement  of  electrodes  in  which  one  active 
electrode  is  surrounded  by  a  number  of  .similar 
electrodes  combined  together  as  a  single  pole.  This 
method  of  recording  seems  to  have  certain  advantages, 
especially  when  precise  localization  and  determination 
of  the  distribution  of  the  action  potential  are  desired 
(58).  The  evoked  potential  diflfers  from  the  so-called 
spontaneous  electrical  changes  in  iTiany  respects, 
notably  the  following,  a)  It  bears  definite  temporal 
relationship  to  the  onset  of  the  stimulus.  In  other 
words,  it  has  a  definite  latent  period  determined  by 
the  conduction  velocity  of  the  nerve  impulses,  the 
conduction  distance  between  the  point  of  stimulation 
and  the  point  of  recording,  the  synaptic  delay  and  the 
number  of  synapses  involved.  In  a  given  system  the 
latency  is  generally  fixed  and  consistent  under 
similar  experimental  conditions.  6)  It  has  a  definite 
pattern  of  response  characteristic  of  a  specific  system 


299 


300 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


which  is  more  or  less  predictable  and  reproducible 
under  similar  conditions,  c)  It  usually  appears  in  a 
circumscribed  area  of  the  central  nervous  system 
where  the  active  tissue  is  located. 

Identification  of  the  evoked  potential  requires 
knowledge  of  anatomical  connections  between  the 
site  of  stimulation  and  the  point  of  recording.  As 
distinct  from  spontaneous  electrical  activity,  po- 
tentials evoked  by  deliberate  stimulation  of  peripheral 
sensory  nerves  are  sharply  localized  in  the  central 
nervous  system.  The  procedure  of  evoked  potential 
registration  thus  becomes  a  useful  tool  for  the  in- 
vestigation of  sensory  pathways.  It  has  been  particu- 
larly fruitful  when  applied  to  the  study  of  cortical 
representation  of  the  auditory,  visual  and  various 
cutaneous  sense  organs. 


l.iimlations  oj  Evoked  Potentials  as   Tools  Jor 
Anatomical  Study 

It  is  not  intended  at  present  to  discuss  in  detail 
sensory  localization  demonstrated  by  the  evoked 
potential  technique.  This  is  discussed  in  the  chapters  of 
this  handbook  dealing  with  the  various  sensory 
mechanisms.  However,  in  order  to  caution  against  the 
misuse  of  the  technique  it  may  be  pertinent  to  men- 
tion briefly  here  some  fundamental  aspects  of  the 
evoked  potential,  especially  those  which  limit  its 
usefulness  in  anatomicophysiological  study.  First,  the 
method  is  valuable  in  determining  the  area  of  sensory 
projection  on  the  cerebral  cortex  only  when  the 
observation  is  made  under  such  conditions  that  the 
cerebral  cortex  is  not  in  an  e.xalted  state  of  excitation, 
if  the  true  sensory  projection  area  is  to  be  determined. 
It  is  an  obvious  fact  that  in  so  complex  an  organiza- 
tion as  the  cerebral  cortex  each  neuron  is  potentially 
related  to  any  other  neuron  through  a  vast  nuinber 
of  chains  of  synaptic  connections.  When  a  given  neu- 
ron is  activated  by  an  afferent  impulse,  almost  any 
other  neuron  may  become  excited  unless  some  re- 
strictive influence  is  exerted  to  curb  the  spread  of  the 
evoked  potential  to  remote  regions  where  ito  afferent 
fibers  terminate  directly.  Second,  the  appearance  of 
an  electrical  change  in  a  given  area  of  the  cerebral 
cortex  does  not  necessarily  indicate  the  presence  of 
neuronal  activity  underlying  that  area.  As  pointed 
out  long  ago  by  Helmholtz,  measurement  of  the 
external  field  of  electrical  current  on  the  surface  of  a 
living  tissue  is  not  adequate  to  ascertain  the  location  of 
the  internal  electromotive  force.  The  electrical 
changes  detected  from  the  surface  of  the  brain  may 
be  derived  from  a  purelv  phssical  process  such  as  the 


potential  field  created  by  the  pa.ssage  of  electrical 
current  along  a  nerve  bathed  in  a  conducting  medium. 
According  to  the  \oiume  conductor  principle  (47,  49), 
when  a  synchronous  volley  of  impulses  passes  along  a 
nerve  embedded  in  a  \olume  of  conducting  medium, 
there  will  appear  in  the  medium  a  travelling  electric 
field  around  the  ner\e.  .Such  a  field  is  caused  by  the 
flow  of  electric  current  from  the  inactive  region  to  the 
depolarized  region  of  the  ner\e.  The  region  occupied 
by  the  nerve  impulse  serves  as  a  fictitious  sink  of 
current  flow^  and  the  regions  lying  ahead  and  behind 
the  impulse  as  fictitious  sources.  Thus,  the  sign  of  the 
action  current  recorded  from  the  active  elements  in 
the  brain  is  negati\c-positi\e  diphasic  at  the  point 
where  the  nerve  impulse  is  initiated,  positive-negative 
diphasic  at  the  point  where  the  conducting  path  ends 
and  positive-negative-positive  triphasic  at  the  middle 
of  the  conducting  path.  At  points  away  from  the 
acti\e  element  the  sign  of  the  detectable  action  po- 
tential will  depend  on  the  position  of  the  electrode 
relative  to  the  direction  and  the  pattern  of  the  iso- 
potential  lines  of  the  traveling  electric  field. 


COMPONENTS  OF  EVOKED    POTENTI.^LS 
AND   THEIR   IDENTIFICATION 

The  action  potential  in  the  brain  evoked  either  by 
electrical  stimulation  of  the  ascending  pathways  or  by 
adequate  stimulation  of  the  sense  organs  consists  of 
two  components,  the  presynaptic  and  the  post- 
synaptic. The  former  indicates  the  arrival  of  impulses 
passing  along  the  axon  and  their  terminals,  and  the 
latter  the  acti\ities  of  the  cell  body  and  dendrites. 
For  the  purpose  of  illustration,  the  evoked  potential 
of  the  sensory  cortex  may  be  taken  as  an  example. 
Following  the  arrival  of  a  volley  of  afferent  impulses 
from  the  thalamus,  the  projection  area  of  the  cerebral 
cortex  gives  rise  to  a  surface-positive  primary  response 
followed  sometimes  by  a  series  of  rhythmic  after- 
discharges.  The  primary  response  is  made  up  of  the 
presynaptic  potential  produced  by  the  activity  of 
thalamocortical  fibers  and  the  postsynaptic  potential 
produced  by  the  discharge  of  intracortical  neurons. 

The  incoming  impulses  from  the  thalamus  are  often 
blended  with,  and  obscured  by,  the  powerful  post- 
synaptic discharge  of  the  cortical  neurons.  This  is 
especially  true  when  the  afiferent  impulses  are  ini- 
tiated by  stimulation  of  the  peripheral  sense  organs 
or  of  the  pathway  far  away  from  the  cerebral  cortex. 
In  that  event,  the  afferent  impulses  usually  arrive  at 
the  cortex  asynchronously  clue  to  temporal  dispersion. 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


301 


In  the  visual  system,  however,  the  radiation  potential 
is  discernible  in  the  cortical  response  to  stimulation  of 
the  optic  nerve  for  two  reasons:  first,  distinct  groups 
of  fibers  according  to  size  are  present  throughout  the 
pathway  from  the  optic  nerve  to  the  optic  radiation; 
and,  second,  there  are  no  intercalated  neurons  in  the 
lateral  geniculate  body  which,  if  present,  would 
destroy  the  synchrony  of  conduction  of  optic  impulses 
and  thus  obliterate  the  characteristic  triple-spike 
radiation  potential.  In  the  auditory  system  as  well  as 
in  the  somesthetic  system  the  presynaptic  component 
of  the  evoked  potential  can  also  be  demonstrated, 
though  less  prominently  than  in  the  visual  system,  if 
the  corresponding  thalamic  nucleus  is  directly 
stimulated  with  a  brief  electric  shock. 

In  spite  of  the  composite  nature  of  the  primary 
response,  the  two  components  can  be  readily  dif- 
ferentiated from  each  other  by  various  experimental 
procedures  as  described  below. 

The  Latent  Period 

The  presynaptic  component  of  the  evoked  po- 
tential, being  the  initial  sign  of  activity,  has  the 
shortest  latency  with  a  value  contingent  upon  the 
fiber  diameter  and  the  conduction  distance  of  a 
given  system.  It  is  readily  identifiable  in  a  system 
composed  of  fibers  of  uniform  size.  The  temporal 
dispersion  of  the  presynaptic  impulses  passing  along 
a  bundle  of  fibers  of  different  sizes  may  make  the  time 
of  arrival  at  the  point  of  recording  vary  over  a  wide 
range  so  that  the  last  impulses  may  overlap  with  the 
postsynaptic  discharge  set  up  by  the  fast  fibers. 
Under  such  circumstances  it  is  impossible  to  dis- 
tinguish presynaptic  from  postsynaptic  activity  merely 
by  the  latency;  only  the  activity  of  the  fastest  fibers  of 
the  group  can  be  ascertained.  In  fact,  such  is  always 
the  case  in  the  cortical  and  subcortical  potentials 
evoked  by  adequate  stimulation  of  peripheral  sense 
organs. 

Another  factor  which  may  seriously  limit  the  ap- 
plicability and  the  value  of  latency  measurement  is 
the  possible  reduction  in  conduction  velocity  of 
impulses  at  nerve  terminals  resulting  from  the  diminu- 
tion of  fiber  diameter.  It  is  not  known  whether  the 
impulses  vanish  instantly  at  the  specialized  pre- 
synaptic endings.  According  to  Barron  &  Matthews 
(4),  and  Lloyd  &  Mclntyre  (46)  a  prolonged  nega- 
tivity persists  at  the  afferent  terminals  of  spinal  dorsal 
root  fibers  and  is  detectable  at  a  considerable  distance 
from  the  active  fibers  as  the  dorsal  root  potential 
DR-IV    in     Lloyd's     terminology.     The    prolonged 


depolarization    of   a    dorsal    root    fiber    following    a 
single   shock   stimulation   can   be   recorded   with   an 

intracellular  microelectrode  (41). 

Efect  oj  Repetitive  Stimulation 

It  has  long  Iseen  known  that  synaptic  transmission 
can  be  blocked  by  stimuli  delivered  in  quick  suc- 
cession. At  certain  rates  of  stimulation  the  amplitude 
of  the  electrical  response  involving  synapses  decreases 
with  each  response,  becoming  succes.sively  smaller 
than  the  preceding  one  until  finally  the  response 
disappears  entirely.  The  repolarization  process  of 
the  membrane  of  the  neuron  .soma  which  receives  the 
presynaptic  excitation  apparently  requires  a  longer 
time  than  the  axon.  The  postsynaptic  neuron  is  not 
al)le  to  respond  to  successively  arriving  impulses 
until  the  recovery  of  its  excitability  becomes  complete. 
The  effect  of  synaptic  block  by  repetitive  stiinulation 
is  especially  pronounced  in  subjects  under  deep 
anesthesia  by  barbiturates.  The  method  has  been 
successfully  used  in  differentiation  of  the  presynaptic 
froin  the  postsynaptic  components  of  the  evoked 
potential  in  the  lateral  geniculate  body  by  Bishop  & 
McLeod  (8).  As  another  example  to  illustrate  the 
differential  effect  of  repetitive  stimulation  on  the 
pre-  and  the  postsynaptic  potentials,  the  electrical 
response  of  the  pyramidal  tract  to  electrical  stimula- 
tion of  the  motor  cortex  may  be  taken.  According  to 
the  study  by  Patton  &  Amassian  (57)  the  pyramidal 
response  to  cortical  stimulation  consists  of  an  early- 
wave  resulting  from  direct  stimulation  of  the  motor 
neurons  and  a  later  wave  resulting  from  the  activity 
of  cortical  internuncial  neurons,  which  is  elicited 
indirectly  or  synaptically.  The  former  can  follow 
repetitive  stimulation  at  frequencies  as  high  as  340  per 
sec.  with  only  slight  reduction  in  amplitude,  whereas 
the  latter  disappears  from  the  response  when  stimulus 
frequency  is  increased  from  44.7  per  sec.  to  127  per 
sec. 

The  presynaptic  response  or  the  response  of  an  axon 
to  direct  electrical  stimulation  is  usually  able  to 
follow  faithfully  the  stimuli  at  a  high  rate  limited  only 
l)y  the  refractory  period.  The  degree  of  the  blocking 
effect  of  repetitive  stimulation  seems  to  increase  with 
the  increase  in  number  of  synapses  involved.  For 
instance,  in  the  first  relay  station  of  the  dorsal  root 
fiijers,  i.e.  the  cuneate  nucleus,  the  postsynaptic 
discharge  of  a  single  neuron  to  repetitive  stimulation 
of  peripheral  sense  organs  or  of  its  nerve  can  follow 
the  rate  of  stimulation  as  high  as  100  per  sec.  without 
substantial  modification  of  either  the  response  ampli- 


302 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tude  or  the  latency-  Increase  in  rate  of  stimulation 
reduces  the  number  of  spikes  of  the  responding;  neuron. 
It  has  been  observed,  though  very  infrequently, 
that  the  cuneate  neuron  may  respond  to  peripheral 
stimulation  at  a  rate  as  high  as  500  per  sec.  (2). 
Evoked  potentials  of  the  thalamic  neurons,  which 
receive  afferent  impulses  after  a  number  of  synaptic 
relays  at  lower  levels  of  the  neural  axis,  cannot  follow 
rates  of  stimulation  even  as  low  as  20  per  sec.  The 
somatic  .sensory  cortex  is  known  to  be  unable  to 
respond  fully  to  peripheral  stimulation  at  a  rate 
higher  than  7  per  sec.  in  animals  under  barbiturate 
anesthesia  (33);  in  other  conditions  the  rate  may  be  as 
high  as  14  per  sec.  (40). 

Ejjcct  (if  Changes  in  Internal  and  External  Milicn 

The  axon  is  generally  known  to  withstand  adverse 
changes  of  the  internal  or  external  milieu  better  than 
the  cell  body  and  dendrites.  In  accordance  with  this 
tenet,  the  postsynaptic  potential  which  insoKes  the 
activity  of  the  latter  structures  has  fjeen  found  to  be 
more  susceptible  to  the  lack  of  oxygen  than  the 
presynaptic  potential.  In  complete  anoxia  produced 
by  asphyxiation  or  by  inhaling  pure  nitrogen,  for 
instance,  the  postsynaptic  potential  can  be  abolished 
in  about  90  sec.  while  the  activity  of  the  presynaptic 
fibers  may  last  for  a  considerably  longer  period  of  time. 
The  greatest  difference  between  the  pre-  and  the 
postsynaptic  potentials,  however,  lies  in  the  rate  of 
recovery  from  anoxia.  Experimental  evidence  shows 
that  the  axonal  component  of  the  cortical  response  to 
stimulation  of  the  medullary  pyramid  begins  to  re- 
cover from  the  effect  of  anoxia  in  about  i  min.  after 
the  readmission  of  oxygen  and  resumes  its  original 
size  in  about  5  min.  The  postsynaptic  component  of 
the  response,  on  the  other  hand,  will  not  reappear 
until  5  or  6  min.  later.  A  complete  recovery  mav  re- 
quire even  10  or  20  min.,  depending  on  how  soon 
oxygen  was  readmitted  (21,  22).  Similarly,  the 
synaptically  elicited  wave  (I-wave^  of  the  pyramidal 
response  to  cortical  stimulation  is  reduced  in  size 
after  70  sec.  of  asphyxia  and  virtually  aljolished  after 
130  sec,  while  the  directly  elicited  di.scharges  (D- 
wave)  persist  Co/)- 

Like  anoxia,  mechanical  pressure,  traumatic  injury 
and  low  temperature  all  depress  the  postsynaptic 
function  sooner  and  more  severely  than  the  pre- 
synaptic activity.  There  are  some  chemicals  such  as 
strychnine  and  tubocurarine  which  may  enhance 
specifically  the  postsynaptic  actisity  without  markedly 
aflfecting  the  presynaptic  potentials  (20,  25). 


It  has  been  observed  that  when  the  cortical  surface 
was  cooled  h\  controlled  refrigeration,  the  functional 
activity  of  dendrites  of  cortical  neurons  was  partially 
blocked  at  temperatures  below  28°C  and  was  coin- 
pletely  abolished  at  22 °C;,  while  the  functional  activity 
of  axon  remained  without  adverse  changes.  From  this 
fact  it  inay  be  inferred  that  the  postsynaptic  potential 
which  invokes  the  process  of  depolarization  of 
dendrites  must  be  affected  Ijy  low  temperature  more 
severely  than  the  potentials  deri\ed  from  the  directly 
excited  axons  (21). 

Anatomical  CUnsiiIeratinns 

In  determining  whether  or  not  a  potential  compo- 
nent is  pre-  or  postsynaptic,  the  anatomical  situation 
must  be  considered  as  a  decisive  factor.  Obviously  one 
cannot  assign  a  potential  as  postsynaptic  if  there  are 
only  directly  excited  fibers  present  in  the  system  in- 
volved. In  the  case  of  antidromic  action  potential  in 
the  optic  ner\e  elicited  by  stimulation  of  the  optic 
tract,  for  instance,  it  is  obviously  not  possible  to  have  a 
postsynaptic  component  in  the  potentials  obtained 
(24).  However,  it  would  not  be  so  easy  to  be  certain 
in  a  central  structure  which  is  embedded  among  a 
complicated  mass  of  \arious  neural  elements.  In  that 
circumstance,  the  characteristics  of  the  recorded 
potential  must  be  taken  into  consideration  together 
with  the  related  anatomical  organization  of  the  system 
concerned.  An  approach  of  this  kind  has  been  adopted 
frequently  in  analysis  of  evoked  cortical  potentials. 
We  may  take  as  an  example  the  microelectrode  study 
of  the  cortical  potential  evoked  by  stimulation  of  the 
ventrolateral  nucleus  of  the  thalamus  (43).  Recordings 
taken  from  different  depths  of  the  cerebral  cortex  in- 
variably show  the  presence  of  positive-negative 
diphasic  spikes  in  the  early  phase  of  the  potential. 
These  spikes  which  have  comparatively  low  voltage 
are  frequently  seen  at  all  le\els  below  0.7  mm.  The 
negative  phase  of  the  spike  increases  as  the  electrode 
is  pushed  deeper  into  the  cortex.  They  can  easily  be 
distinguished  from  the  high  voltage  spikes  derived 
from  the  cell  bodies.  The  short  latency  and  the  brief 
duration  of  the  spikes  makes  it  certain  that  they  are 
from  the  presynaptic  thalamocortical  fibers  which  are 
known  to  terminate  mainly  in  the  fourth  layer  of  the 
cortex  located  about  0.7  mm  beneath  the  cortical 
surface  in  the  cat.  Alignment  of  simultaneous  re- 
cordings from  the  cortical  surface  with  a  gross 
electrode  and  those  from  the  depth  when  a  micro- 
electrode  shows  a  temporal  coincidence  of  the  small 
diphasic  spikes  and  the  usual  elevations  of  the  po- 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


303 


tential  of  surface  recordings  which  have  often  been 
designated  as  radiation  potentials. 

The  cell  body  discharges  are  usually  spikes  of  high 
voltage  and  are  of  quite  long  duration.  The  diflference 
in  duration  between  the  potentials  of  a  single  axon 
and  of  a  single  cell  body  has  been  established  through 
the  microelectrode  studies  on  spinal  ganglia  (65),  on 
the  spinal  cord  (68),  on  the  lateral  geniculate  body 
and  on  the  cerebral  cortex  (66).  According  to  Wood- 
bury &  Patton  (68)  the  duration  of  the  spike  is  about 
0.6  msec,  for  the  axon  and  i  msec,  for  the  cell  body- 
Tasaki  et  al.  (66)  put  the  values  as  i  msec,  or  less  for 
axonal  response  and  1.5  to  3  m.sec.  for  the  cell  body 
respon.se.  Frank  &  Fuortes  found  the  respective  values 
for  dorsal  root  fibers  and  cell  body  of  the  spinal 
motoneurons  to  be  0.6  msec,  and  1.6  msec,  respec- 
tively (35). 


NEUR.'^L  MECH.^NISMS  FOR  THE  EL.^BOR.JiTION  OF 
EVOKED   CORTICAL    POTENTI.ALS 

The  generation  of  the  radiation  spikes  of  the  evoked 
potential  is  a  relatively  simple  problem.  It  is  a 
generally  accepted  opinion  that  the  initial  sharp 
spikes  with  short  latency  represent  the  arrival  of 
afferent  impulses  which  are  purely  presynaptic  in 
nature.  Sufficient  evidence  is  available  that  the  main 
part  of  the  surface  positive  wave  of  the  prima r\ 
response  on  which  the  presynaptic  spikes  may  be 
superimposed  is  made  up  of  the  discharges  of  the 
cortical  neurons. 

Types   oj  Neural   Elements    Im'nlvrd  and 
Their  Mode   of  Action 

As  to  exactly  what  cortical  elements  are  responsible 
for  the  production  of  this  potential  and  what  is  the 
mechanism  by  which  an  afferent  \olley  initiates  the 
discharge  of  those  elements,  remain  points  to  be 
elucidated.  Since  there  are  few  available  data  con- 
cerning the  roles  played  by  different  types  of  cortical 
elements,  the  postulation  of  a  mechanism  for  the 
genesis  of  the  evoked  cortical  potential  must  be  made 
on  the  basis  of  the  histological  organization  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  and  the  estaljlished  principles  of 
electrophysiology.  In  view  of  the  fact  that  the  basic 
pattern  of  the  cortical  response  to  afferent  impulses 
appears  remarkaijly  constant  throughout  the  sensory 
cortex  and  that  the  sensory  cortex  in\'ariably  receives 
specific  thalamic  afferent  fillers  and  possesses  a  verv 
well  developed  granular  layer,  these  latter  two  struc- 


tural characteristics  must  be  taken  into  consideration 
in  offering  any  explanation  of  the  e\oked  cortical 
potential. 

The  specific  afferent  fibers  arising  from  the  thala- 
mus are  known  to  terminate  mainly  in  the  fourth 
layer  by  a  rich  plexus  of  repeatedly  arborized  endings. 
Afferent  impulses  coming  along  these  fibers  make  their 
first  synaptic  contact  with  Golgi  type  II  cells  in  the 
fourth  layer.  Golgi  type  II  cells  are  characterized 
by  the  presence  of  short  axons  terminating  in  profuse 
arborizations  in  a  localized  region  surrounding  the 
parent  cell  body.  Their  dendrites  are  rather  few  and 
poorly  de\eloped.  By  virtue  of  their  anatomical  char- 
acteristics they  are  not  able  to  transmit  impulses  to 
distant  regions  but  serve  as  amplifiers  by  which  the 
afferent  impulses  are  reinforced.  They  are  un- 
doubtedly indispensable  for  the  elaboration  of  the 
evoked  cortical  potentials.  However,  since  there  is  no 
definite  orientation  of  the  conducting  structure  of 
these  cells  and  the  electric  field  created  by  the  dis- 
charge of  these  cells  is  a  closed  type  (48),  their  activitv 
cannot  be  recorded  as  any  sizable  potential  from  the 
cortical  surface.  The  consequence  of  the  discharge  of 
Golgi  type  II  cells  in  la>er  IV  is  probably  to  activate 
the  star  p\  ramids  and  the  star  cells  in  the  same  layer 
which  in  turn  activate  the  numerous  medium  and 
small  p\raniids  which  eventually  depolarize  the  large 
pxramids  in  the  fifth  and  sixth  layers.  The  large 
p\  ramids  send  out  efferent  axons  to  some  other  parts  of 
the  central  nervous  s\stem. 


The  Initiation  of  Postsynaptic  Impulses 
in  Pyramidal  Neurons 

The  detectable  surface-positive  potential  can  be 
reasonably  assigned  to  the  propagation  of  nerve 
impulses  along  the  vertically  oriented  apical  dendrites 
of  different  sized  cortical  pyramids.  Under  normal 
conditions  the  depolarization  process  of  cortical 
pyramids  resulting  from  a  supraliminal  synaptic 
excitation  is  apt  to  start  at  the  somatic  membrane 
around  the  cell  body  rather  than  at  the  terminal 
portion  of  the  dendrites.  According  to  a  recent 
postulation  (17,  23)  the  pericorpuscular  synaptic 
knobs  constitute  the  inost  effective  apparatus  for  the 
initiation  of  a  postsynaptic  discharge,  whereas  the 
subliminal  excitation  of  paradendritic  synapses  can 
produce  only  electrotonic  changes  and  so  modifv  the 
state  of  excitability  of  the  neuron.  The  paradendritic 
synapses,  because  of  their  lower  density  of  distribution 
and  their  special  manner  of  contact  with  the  next 
neuron,   are   believed   to   be   inadequate   to  effect  a 


304 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


postsynaptic  neuronal  discharge  under  ordinary 
conditions.  It  is  well  known  that  the  dendritic  shafts  of 
the  large  pyramidal  neurons  in  the  fifth  and  sixth 
layers  of  the  sensory  cortex  do  not  give  off  branches  in 
the  fourth  layer  where  the  terminals  of  the  thalamo- 
cortical fibers  and  the  aggregations  of  Golgi  type  II 
cells  arc  located.  The  only  contact  between  the  large 
pyramidal  cells  and  the  afferent  elements  is  made 
through  the  comparatively  few  paradendritic  synapses 
which  are  not  sufficient  to  bring  about  a  postsvnaptic 
discharge.  It  is  most  unlikely  that  the  afferent  fibers 
from  the  thalamus  and  Golgi  type  II  cells  in  layer  IV 
can  ever  directly  acti\ate  the  large  pyramidal  cells. 
The  discharge  of  the  large  pyramidal  cells  on  arrival 
of  an  afferent  volley  of  impulses  must  be  achieved 
through  the  action  of  pericorpuscular  synapses  sup- 
plied by  small  and  medium  pyramidal  cells. 

Apical  Dendrites  and  Electrical  Signs 
of  the  Evoked  Potential 

From  the  point  of  view  set  forth  above,  the  surface- 
positivity  of  the  primary  response  of  the  evoked  corti- 
cal potential  can  be  reasonably  explained.  When  the 
pyramidal  cells  are  indirectly  activated  by  afferent 
impulses  through  chains  of  internuncial  neurons 
including  Golgi  type  II  cells,  star  pyramids  and  small 
and  medium  pyramidal  cells  which  make  pericor- 
pu.scular  synapsis  with  the  large  pyramids,  the  post- 
synaptic impulses  initiated  at  the  cell  body  will  serve 
as  a  sink  and  the  apical  dendrites  as  a  source  of  the 
current  flow.  The  record  of  such  electrical  change 
taken  from  the  cortical  surface  will  be  a  positive  wave. 
As  soon  as  the  impulses  arrive  at  the  apical  dendritic 
ple.xus  at  the  cortical  surface,  the  electrical  sign  of  the 
potential  will  be  reversed.  The  surface-negative  wave 
following  the  positive  deflection  of  the  primary 
response  may  be  accounted  for,  at  least  in  part,  on 
this  basis.  As  pointed  out  previously  (14)  this  interpre- 
tation does  not  exclude  the  possibility  that  other 
cortical  elements  participate  in  the  elaboration  of  the 
surface  negative  deflection.  In  fact,  the  neurons 
situated  in  the  upper  layers  of  the  cortex  must  also 
be  involved. 

The  conduction  velocity  of  impulses  passing  along 
dendrites  is  less  than  2  m  per  sec,  which  is  many 
times  slower  than  that  of  impulses  passing  along  axons. 
There  is  also  a  decremental  reduction  in  velocity  as 
the  impulses  are  propagated  from  the  proximal  part 
to  the  terminal  regions  of  the  dendrites  (21).  The 
long  duration  of  the  surface-positive  wave  of  the 
evoked   potential   gives  every  indication   of  being  a 


manifestation  of  dendritic  activity.  It  is  more  than 
probable  that  the  process  underlying  the  surface- 
positive  wave  and  the  following  negativity  lie  mainly 
in  the  apical  dendrites  of  different  groups  of  cortical 
pyramids.  The  deeply  situated  basal  dendrites  of  the 
pyramidal  cells  are  mostly  oriented  toward  the  sub- 
cortical white  matter  or  more  or  less  horizontallv.  In 
other  words,  they  are  arranged  in  a  direction  roughly 
opposite  to  that  taken  by  the  apical  dendrites.  Thus, 
during  the  discharge  of  the  pyramidal  cells  the  po- 
tential changes  originating  from  the  basal  dendrites 
must  be  greatly  neutralized  by  the  divergently 
propagating  potentials  along  the  overwhelmingly 
dominant  apical  dendrites,  if  the  potentials  are 
recorded  from  a  lead  on  the  cortical  surface. 

Micrnelectrode  Findings  Concerning  the  Mechanism 
of  Impulse  Initialiiin  in  Single  .Neurons 

Without  going  into  a  detailed  discussion  of  the 
fundamental  mechanism  by  which  the  propagated 
impulse  is  initiated  hw  electrical  or  synaptic  excitation, 
it  may  be  relevant  to  mention  here  a  few  experimental 
facts  which  seem  to  characterize  the  synaptically 
evoked  potentials  as  contrasted  with  the  antidromi- 
cally  produced  potentials.  As  revealed  by  intracellular 
microelectrode  recordings,  the  discharge  of  a  neuron 
elicited  by  synaptic  excitation  of  the  cell  body  or 
dendrites  is  characteristically  different  from  that 
elicited  by  stimulation  of  its  axon.  The  synaptically 
produced  discharge  as  well  as  the  spontaneous  firing 
of  a  neuron  is  usually  preceded  by  a  slowly  rising 
positive  deflection  upon  which  the  sharp  spike  rides 
when  the  discharge  threshold  is  reached.  On  sub- 
liminal stimulation  only  the  small  deflection  will  be 
present.  Such  preliminary  potentials  have  been 
observed  in  spinal  motoneurons  (11),  in  the  thalamus 
(60)  and  in  Betz  cells  of  the  motor  cortex  (59).  It  has 
sometimes  been  called  the  *  synaptic  potential".  Since 
it  may  be  present  without  necessarily  involving 
synaptic  transfer  of  impulses  and  since  the  term  also 
describes  the  potentials  recorded  by  other  means,  it 
has  been  suggested  to  adopt  the  noncommittal  term 
'  prepotential'  in  its  place  (68). 

The  fact  that  the  prepotential  is  present  mostly  in 
the  evoked  or  spontaneously  occurring  responses 
which  involve  the  activity  of  cell  body  and  dendrites 
but  not  in  the  antidromic  responses  seems  to  suggest 
that  activity  of  the  dendrites  plays  an  important  role 
in  initiation  of  the  spike  discharge.  In  this  respect 
Eyzaguirre  &  Kuffler's  study  on  single  neurons  of  the 
lobster  and  cravfish  has  thrown  much  light  on  the 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


305 


mechanism  of  impulse  initiation  in  a  neuron  (30-32). 
Eyzaguirre  &  Kuffler  found  that  suljliminal  excita- 
tion of  the  dendrite  by  mechanical  stretch  of  the 
muscle  in  which  the  dendrites  are  imbedded  produces 
a  reduction  of  the  membrane  potential  of  the  cell  body 
through  the  electrotonic  effect.  A  propagated  discharge 
takes  place  only  when  the  membrane  potential  is 
reduced  to  a  certain  level.  From  this  it  appears  that 
the  prepotential  of  the  evoked  response  may  represent 
nothing  but  a  partial  depolarization  of  the  resting 
membrane  potential  resulting  from  subliminal  excita- 
tion of  dendrites  by  bombardments  of  presynaptic 
nerve  endings.  Antidromic  discharge  of  a  neuron 
apparently  calls  for  no  such  build-up  of  the  ex- 
citability level  as  a  prerequisite  and  is  therefore  devoid 
of  the  characteristic  small  prepotential  often  seen  in 
responses  produced  by  synaptic  action. 


.\FTER-DISCH.^RGES 

The  term  '  after-discharge'  has  been  conventionally 
employed  to  describe  the  epileptiform  discharges  of 
neurons  following  strong  tetanic  stimulation  which 
persist  long  after  the  cessation  of  stimulation.  Since 
accumulated  experimental  data  show  that  repetitive 
bursts  occur  not  only  after  tetanic  stimulation  but  also 
after  a  single  shock  applied  to  the  system,  we  will 
designate  all  the  discharges  which  outlast  the  duration 
of  stimulation,  tetanic  or  single  shock,  as  after- 
discharges,  in  the  very  loose  sense  of  the  terminology. 

For  the  sake  of  convenience  in  discussion,  after- 
discharges  can  be  classified  into  three  types:  /)  repeti- 
tive firings  of  single  elements  which  are  self-main- 
tained without  the  participation  of  other  elements  in 
their  production;  2)  persistent  local  after-discharges 
involving  the  activity  of  closely  situated  intrinsic 
neurons  which  form  short  neuronal  circuits;  and 
j)  periodic  discharges  involving  re\erberating  activi- 
ties of  a  closed  neuronal  circuit  formed  by  long  chains 
of  neurons  connecting  remotely  separated  structures. 

Repetitive  Firing  of  Individual  Neurons 

Microelectrode  recordings  from  the  thalamic  and 
the  cortical  neurons  show  that  a  .single  neuron  fires 
several  times  in  response  to  an  afferent  volley.  To  a 
stimulus  at  threshold  strength  a  neuron  generally 
responds  by  giving  rise  to  a  single  spike.  As  the  stim- 
ulus increases  in  strength,  the  number  of  the  spikes 
increases  correspondingly.  It  has  been  reported  that  a 
thalamic  unit  may  fire  seven  times  in  quick  succession 


in  response  to  optimal  stimulation  of  the  skin  re- 
ceptors (60).  A  single  neuron  in  the  reticular  forma- 
tion, for  instance,  may  give  rise  to  a  train  of  as  many  as 
20  spikes  in  response  to  a  single  stimulus  (3).  In  so  far 
as  the  length  of  the  train  is  concerned,  natural  ade- 
quate stimulation  of  the  sense  organs  seems  to  be  more 
effective  than  electrical  stimulation  of  the  nerve.  In 
electrical  stimulation,  once  the  threshold  is  reached 
further  increase  in  intensity  seems  to  be  rather  in- 
effective in  inducing  any  greater  responses.  On  the 
contrary,  strong  stimulation  may  inhibit  the  suc- 
cessive spikes.  This  is  true  for  somesthetic,  auditorv, 
visual  and  olfactory  systems. 

In  a  long  train  of  repetiti\e  discharges  at  high 
frequency,  the  first  spike  is  usually  the  largest  in 
amplitude  and  the  second  the  smallest.  The  rest  of  the 
spikes  following  the  second  gradually  increase  in  size 
until  they  approach,  but  rarely  become  as  large  as, 
the  first  one.  The  deficit  of  the  .successive  spikes  is 
probably  caused  either  by  the  incomplete  repolariza- 
tion of  the  neuron  membrane  following  a  forceful  dis- 
charge, or  by  the  postexcitatory  depression  associated 
with  the  process  of  hyperpolarization  of  the  mem- 
brane. 

The  repetitive  discharge  of  a  single  neuron  is 
believed  to  be  a  self-sustained  process  which  is 
initiated  only  by  the  afferent  volley  but  is  not  the 
result  of  repeated  bombardments  by  presynaptic 
impulses.  This  belief  is  based  on  the  observation  that  a 
small  deflection  immediately  preceding  the  first  spike 
of  a  train,  which  can  be  interpreted  as  a  presynaptic 
potential,  is  present  only  once  at  the  beginning  of  the 
train.  No  similar  potential  has  ever  been  observed 
preceding  the  successive  spikes.  The  individual  spikes 
in  a  train  do  not  correspond  to  the  successive  arrivals 
of  presynaptic  volleys  of  impulses.  Therefore,  the 
repetiti\e  firing  cannot  be  regarded  as  resulting  from 
the  repetitise  arrival  of  presynaptic  impulses.  Single 
nerve  elements  are  endowed  with  the  capacity  to 
discharge  repetitively  in  response  to  a  stimulus.  It  has 
been  repeatedly  demonstrated  that  following  a  single 
shock  applied  to  the  dorsal  root  a  burst  of  four  or  five 
unit  spikes  can  be  recorded  in  the  dorsal  column 
where,  due  to  the  absence  of  intercalated  neurons, 
synapses  are  not  in\'olved. 

The  intimate  nature  of  the  .self-generating  mechan- 
ism of  after-discharge  inside  the  neuron  is  not  known. 
Burns  (13)  suggests  that  the  repetitive  firing  of  a 
cortical  neiu-on  following  stimulation  is  due  to  the 
difference  in  recovery  rates  of  resting  membrane 
potentials  at  the  two  ends  of  a  neuron  such  that  one 
end  is  repolarized  more  slowly  than  the  other.   By 


3o6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


virtue  of  the  differential  rates  of  depolarization  a 
neuron  is  able  to  fire  repetitively,  resembling  the 
oscillatory  discharge  and  recharge  of  two  thyratron 
tubes  with  two  different-valued  condensers  in  the 
circuit.  This  concept  of  Burns  is  apparently  derived 
from  his  observation  that  after-discharge  of  skeletal 
muscle  fibers  may  be  caused  by  treatment  with 
decamethonium  iodide,  which  is  believed  to  pre\ent 
the  end  plate  membrane  from  repolarizing  as  rapidly 
as  the  neighboring  membrane  of  the  muscle  fiber. 

Local  AJter-Discharges   Involving   Intrinsic 
Neuronal   Circuits 

Although  individual  neurons  are  capable  of  dis- 
charging repetitively  in  response  to  a  single  stimulus, 
long  lasting  activities  in  the  central  nervous  system  are 
mostly  manifestations  of  neuronal  discharges  resulting 
from  a  self-re-exciting  mechanism  involving  numerous 
neurons  arranged  in  closed  circuits  in  the  same  struc- 
ture. 

As  demonstrated  by  Burns  (12),  an  isolated  slab  of 
cerebral  cortex  is  able  to  discharge  following  a  single 
shock  applied  to  the  cortical  surface.  Such  discharges 
may  last  for  many  minutes  or  even  hours  in  some 
instances  and  are  detectable  not  only  in  the  circum- 
scribed area  directly  under  stimulation  but  also  in 
regions  at  some  distance  from  it.  They  are  evidently 
not  the  repetitive  firings  of  the  directly  stimulated 
neurons  but  the  responses  of  neurons  synaptically 
excited  through  neuronal  circuits.  After-discharges  of 
this  kind  usually  develop  increasing  intensity  and 
then  suddenly  stop  altogether  at  the  climax.  They 
may  resume  the  activity  after  a  brief  pause.  In  such  a 
case,  the  activity  of  individual  neurons  apparently 
depends  on  the  arrival  of  impulses  from  some  other 
neurons  in  the  circuit  for  re-excitation.  To  perpetuate 
the  activity,  the  circulating  impulses  must  be  main- 
tained above  the  liminal  strength  and  arrive  at  the 
next  neuron  at  an  opportune  moment  when  the 
excitability  of  the  neuron  is  favorable.  The  abrupt 
cessation  or  suspension  of  the  after-discharge  at  its 
climax  is  probably  due  to  the  postexcitatory  depres- 
sion of  some  neurons  in  the  circuit  which  fail  to 
respond  to  the  arriving  impulses  so  that  the  circuit  is 
broken.  The  self-re-exciting  circuits  are  present  in 
every  part  of  the  central  nervous  system  where  inter- 
nuncial  neurons  exist.  Many  neurons,  especially 
those  whose  axons  are  short  but  have  numerous 
collaterals,  constitute  the  main  source  for  the  elabora- 
tion of  local  after-discharges. 

The  acti\-ation  of  the  internuncial  neurons  through 


collaterals    has    been   demonstrated    in    the   cerebral 
cortex.    The    action    potential    of   the    motor   cortex 
produced  by  a  single  shock  stimulation  of  the  medul- 
lary pyramid  consists  of  the  initial  deflections  with 
short    latency   and    in   addition   a    rather   prolonged 
discharge  which  has  a  latency  of  14  to  16  msec.   It 
cannot  be  interpreted  as  antidromic  activity  of  the 
directh'   stimulated   large   p\ramidal    neurons.    Such 
later  components  of  the  antidromic  cortical  potential 
are  variable,  labile  and  more  susceptible  to  the  action 
of  anoxia,  specific  drugs  (strychnine  for  instance)  and 
tetanic    stimulation — showing   characteristics    of   the 
responses   invoking   synapses.   When   two   successive 
stimuli  are  applied  to  the  medullary  pyramid  at  short 
intervals,  the  cortical  response  to  the  second  is  usually 
blocked.  The  temporal  course  of  the  recovery  process 
is  similar  to  that  of  orthodromically  evoked  potentials. 
Unit  activity  of  the  internuncial  neurons  participating 
in  the  development  of  such  activity  can  be  recorded 
from   different   strata    of  the   cortex   with   a   micro- 
electrode.  Perhaps  the  most  interesting  is  the  fact  that 
the  large  pyramidal  neurons  whose  axons  ha\e  been 
stimulated  originally  can  be  re-excited  synaptically  by 
their   own    collaterals.    The   discharge   of   the   same 
pvramidal    neuron    resulting    from    the    internuncial 
activity  is  detectable  from  the  point  on  the  medullary 
pvramid  where   the  single  shock  stimulus  has  been 
first  applied.  Thus,  the  action  of  the  self-re-exciting 
circuit  is  completed.  The  particular  significance  of  the 
collateral  acti\ity  of  the  pyramidal  fibers  lies  in  the 
fact    that    they   constitute    a    part    of   the    feed-l^ack 
mechanism  in  the  cerebral  cortex  by  which  a  message 
is  sent  back  to  the  original  dispatcher  for  modification 
of  the   subsequent   responses.    If  the   feed-back    im- 
pulses  are   sufficiently   strong   and   arrive   when   the 
brain  excitability  is  in  the  most  favorable  condition, 
it  is  even   possible   to  initiate  a  rhythmic   after-dis- 
charge of  the  efferent  neurons.  It  is  believed  that  the 
prolonged     epileptiform     after-discharges     following 
strong  stimulation  of  the  motor  cortex  and  the  seizures 
in  pathological  cases  are  produced,  at  least  in  part,  by 
a  feed-back  mechanism  through  the  collaterals  in  the 
closed  chains  ot  neurons. 

Rhythmic  After-Discharges  Involving  Long  Neuronal 
Circuits:  Corticothalamic  Reverheratory  Activity 

The  primary  response  of  the  sensory  cortex  to  an 
afferent  volley  is  often  followed  by  a  train  of  regularly 
spaced  surface-positive  waves  with  intervals  ranging 
from  50  to  150  msec.  (14).  The  frequency  of  the 
repetitive  waves  seems  to  l)e  independent  of  the  stimu- 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


307 


lus  Strength  but  varies  with  the  state  of  anesthesia  and 
the  area  from  which  the  observations  are  made.  The 
repetitive  discharges  evoked  by  afferent  impulses 
differ  from  the  spontaneous  waves  in  many  respects, 
though  they  may  happen  to  have  the  same  frequency. 
The  evoked  repetitive  discharges  are  always  surface- 
positive  waves  whereas  the  spontaneous  activity  of 
cortical  neurons  is  not  necessarily  so.  The  latter 
appears  to  be  regulated  to  some  extent  by  the  intra- 
laminar  nuclear  groups  of  the  thalamus  (39);  but  the 
evoked  periodic  after-discharges  are  not  affected  by 
surgical  removal  or  electrical  stimulation  of  the  massa 
intermedia  (14). 

The  presence  of  the  evoked  periodic  discharges  is 
dependent  upon  the  integrity  of  the  pathways  between 
the  cerebral  cortex  and  the  thalamic  nucleus  con- 
cerned. The  periodic  wa\es  recorded  from  the  cor- 
responding thalamic  nucleus  are  similar  in  pattern  to 
those  observed  from  the  sensory  cortex  and  can  be 
abolished  by  removal  of  the  cortex.  Likewise,  repeti- 
tive discharge  of  the  same  kind  can  be  evoked  by 
direct  stimulation  of  the  cortical  surface  and  abolished 
jjy  the  destruction  of  the  corresponding  thklamic 
structure  or  by  interruption  of  the  thalamocortical 
connections.  From  these  experimental  facts  it  is  sug- 
gested therefore  that  the  specific  periodic  after- 
discharges  following  afferent  stimulation  represent  the 
activity  of  the  reverberating  circuit  between  the 
sensory  cortex  and  the  thalamus.  It  is  as.sumed  that  a 
volley  of  afferent  impulses  from  the  thalamus,  after 
arriving  at  the  cortex,  will  return  to  the  thalamic 
nucleus  and  ascend  again  to  the  cortex  to  start 
another  cycle  of  activity  along  a  closed  chain  of 
neurons. 

It  is  believed  that  the  general  periodic  wa\es  ob- 
served in  the  central  nervous  system  can  be  due  to 
many  causes.  The  activity  of  reverberating  circuits  is 
only  one  of  many  possible  mechanisms  underlying  the 
periodic  waves.  It  would  be  a  mistake  to  regard  all 
kinds  of  rhythmic  discharges  as  being  due  to  the 
activity  of  reverberating  circuits.  The  evoked  repeti- 
tive discharge  in  the  sensory  cortex  must  be  dis- 
tinguished from  the  spontaneous  rhythmic  waves 
which  sometimes  present  themselves  in  such  a  manner 
as  to  confu.se  or  mislead  the  obser\er.  It  is  true  that  in 
unanesthetized  animals  or  in  animals  anesthetized 
with  chloralose  the  rhythmic  discharges  following  a 
single  sound  stimulus  do  occur  in  the  medial  genicu- 
late body  after  decortication  (36).  However,  as 
pointed  out  by  Galambos,  rhythmic  waves  al.so  occur 
spontaneously  without  deliberate  stimulation.  It  is 
obviously  a  mechanism  entirely  different  from  that 


underlying  the  specific  corticothalamic  reverberating 
waves  which,  under  experimental  conditions,  can  be 
evoked  only  by  an  afferent  volley  from  the  thalamus 
or  by  direct  cortical  stimulation.  So  specific  is  this 
response  that  corticothalamic  waves  hav-e  never  been 
obtained  by  stimulation  of  a  symmetrical  point  on  the 
opposite  cortex  although  the  callosal  response  itself 
may  be  a  very  intense  one  (18,  19).  The  failure  of  a 
callosal  volley  to  initiate  the  repetitive  discharges  of 
the  sensory  cortex  at  the  same  cortical  locus  where  the 
thalamic  volley  can  do  so  very  well  seems  to  provide 
strong  evidence  that  the  appearance  of  the  repetitive 
waves  is  dependent  on  the  presence  of  a  specific 
neuronal  circuit  rather  than  being  representative  of 
mere  local  after-discharge  of  an  unorganized  ag- 
gregate of  neurons  having  autorhythmic  properties 

One  of  the  main  difficulties  in  interpretation  of  the 
specific  periodic  after-discharges  according  to  the 
hypothesis  of  corticothalamic  re\erberation  is  perhaps 
the  long  interval  (45  to  1 50  msec.)  between  the  con- 
secutive waves  which  appears  to  be  of  too  great  a 
duration  to  be  accounted  for  solely  by  the  time  neces- 
sary for  the  impulses  to  travel  along  the  neuronal 
circuit  between  the  thalamus  and  the  cortex.  A  pos- 
sible explanation  is  that  the  surface-positive  re- 
verberating waves,  like  the  positive  component  of  the 
primary  response,  presumably  consist  of  synchronous 
discharges  of  cortical  neurons  triggered  by  the  recur- 
rent thalamic  volleys  but  not  the  afferent  impulses 
themselves.  The  latter  may  not  be  detectable  from 
the  cortical  surface.  Because  of  the  \ariation  in  size  of 
fibers  interconnecting  the  thalamus  and  the  cortex  and 
the  variation  in  number  of  synapses  interposed  in  the 
circuit,  the  degree  of  temporal  dispersion  of  the 
circulation  of  the  reverberating  impulses  along  the 
circuit  must  result  in  a  continuous  train  instead  of 
intermittent  waves.  It  is  probable  that  among  the 
returning  impulses  only  those  which  arri\e  at  certain 
phases  of  the  excitability  cycle  of  the  cortical  neurons 
are  capable  of  initiating  synchronous  discharges  of 
these  neurons,  and  the  others  which  arrive  at  the 
cortex  during  the  period  of  postexcitatory  depression 
rendered  by  the  previous  wave  will  not  cause  excita- 
tion. Therefore,  the  interval  between  the  reverberat- 
ing waves  is  probably  determined  by  the  state  of 
excitability  of  the  cortical  neurons  at  the  time  of 
action,  rather  than  by  the  conduction  time  and  the 
number  of  synapses  in  the  circuit.  Thus,  it  becomes 
evident  that  the  activity  of  the  corticothalamic 
reverberating  circuit  cannot  l^e  taken  as  a  simple 
circulating  of  impulses  along  a  closed  chain  of  neurons. 

According  to  Bremer  (9)  the  initiation  and  main- 


3o8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tenance  of  the  periodic  \\a\es  in  the  cerei)ral  cortex, 
although  considered  by  him  to  be  fundamentally  a 
manifestation  of  autorhythmicity  of  neurons,  neces- 
sitate a  minimal  influx  of  corticipetal  impulses.  The 
requirement  of  this  minimal  number  of  afferent 
impulses  for  the  production  of  periodic  cortical  waves 
is  in  some  respects  close  to  the  essential  concept  under- 
lying the  proposed  mechanism  of  corticothalamic 
reverberation.  The  dependence  of  the  so-called 
spontaneous  activity  of  cortical  neurons  on  activation 
by  afferent  impuLses  has  been  con\incingly  pro\'ed  by 
Burns  (12).  He  demonstrated  that  the  spontaneous 
activity  of  isolated  slabs  of  the  cerebral  cortex  did  not 
ensue  unless  a  bridge  was  left  connected  with  the 
rest  of  the  brain.  However,  such  slabs  may  exhibit 
periodic  activity  if  kept  normalK-  oxygenated  (42). 


excit.IlBIlity  ch.i,nges  .accompanying  and 
following  the  evoked  potential 

Like  peripheral  nerves  or  other  excitable  tissues  the 
aggregate  of  neurons  in  the  central  nerxous  system, 
after  being  activated  either  by  direct  or  synaptic 
stimulation,  undergoes  a  cycle  of  excitability  change 
consisting  of  a  refractory  phase  and  a  recovery  phase. 
After  recovery,  it  may  go  into  another  period  of 
secondary  depression  during  which  the  neurons  fail  to 
respond  or  respond  with  less  vigor.  Uniquely  in  the 
sensory  cortex,  a  periodic  variation  in  excitability  may 
deselop  as  a  result  of  the  corticothalamic  re\erberat- 
insf  activity. 


RefraclDiy  Periods 

The  alisolute  refractory  period  following  the  re- 
sponse of  the  somesthetic  corte.x  to  an  aflferent  stimulus 
was  about  8  msec,  and  the  total  recovery  time  was  i  7 
msec,  as  determined  in  monkeys  under  ether  anes- 
thesia. Barbiturates  have  the  effect  of  lengthening  the 
recovery  time.  For  example.,  under  pentobarbital 
anesthesia  the  absolute  and  the  relative  refractory 
periods  were  found  to  be  of  the  order  of  25  to  50  msec, 
and  87  to  144  msec,  respectively  (52,  55).  Forbes  & 
Morison  (33)  found  that  the  amplitude  of  the  primary 
response  of  the  somesthetic  cortex  was  reduced  to  50  to 
70  per  cent  of  its  initial  value  when  the  sciatic  nerve 
of  a  cat  was  stimulated  at  a  frequency  of  5  to  7  per 
sec,  implying  that  the  relative  refractory  period  was 
much  longer  than  the  value  obtained  by  Marshall 
et  al.  (55).  Forbes  et  al.  (34)  later  reported  that  stimula- 
tion of  the  sciatic  nerve  at  a  frequency  of  60  per  sec. 


produced  no  detectable  cortical  response  after  the  re- 
sponse to  the  first  stimulus  of  the  series.  They  also 
reported  the  decrease  in  size  of  the  primary  response  to 
repetitive  stimuli  at  a  frequency  of  5.5  per  sec.  and  the 
phenomenon  of  alternative  response  to  repetitive 
stimuli  delivered  at  the  rate  of  14  per  sec. 

The  values  of  the  absolute  and  relative  refractory 
periods  in  the  \isual  cortex  of  man  and  animals  as 
determined  by  Gastaut  et  al.  (37)  were  20  and  40 
msec,  respectiveh'.  According  to  Tunturi  (67),  the 
absolute  refractory  period  in  the  auditory  cortex  to  a 
click  lasts  20  to  100  msec,  and  the  duration  of  the 
relative  refractory  period  is  100  to  250  msec.  A  glance 
at  the  figures  obtained  by  various  investigators  makes 
one  immediately  realize  the  impossibility  of  finding 
standard  values  for  these  events  since  the  experimental 
conditions  which  determine  the  results  are  extremely 
variable.  Among  the  more  important  factors  affecting 
the  excitability  of  the  brain  are  the  anesthetics  used  in 
the  experiment  (40),  the  depth  of  anesthesia  during 
which  the  observations  are  made  (29,  33.  62),  the 
arterial  pressure  (6),  the  moisture  (54),  the  tempera- 
ture, etc.  The  level  of  tonic  reticular  activity  is  also  a 
factor  of  major  importance  (40). 

The  effect  of  barbiturates  on  the  excitability  of  the 
nervous  system  is  particularly  interesting.  It  has  been 
suggested  that  barbiturates  act  selecti\ely  on  inter- 
nuncial  neurons  in  the  corte.x  rather  than  on  the 
afTercnt  pathway.  The  suggestion  remains  to  be 
reconciled  with  the  fact  that  under  barbiturate 
anesthesia  the  primary  response  of  the  exoked  poten- 
tial is  little  affected  as  compared  with  the  marked 
suppression  of  the  spontaneous  cortical  waves.  As  is 
known,  the  primary  response  of  the  ex-oked  cortical 
potential  consists  largely  of  the  actixity  of  cortical 
internuncial  ncin-ons. 

The  total  period  of  refractoriness  of  the  auditory 
cortex  following  a  direct  electric  shock  was  about  44 
msec.  The  absolute  refractory  period  was  estimated  as 
about  7  or  8  msec.  This  value  as  compared  with  that 
of  the  peripheral  nerve  or  with  that  of  the  individual 
neurons  in  the  central  nervous  system  is  indeed  very 
large.  The  absolute  refractory  period  of  a  nerve  fiber 
is  known  to  occupy  about  the  same  time  as  the  rising 
phase  of  the  action  potential,  xvhich  usuallx'  does  not 
exceed  i  msec.  It  has  been  frequently  ob.serxed  that 
the  minimal  interval  between  successix-e  spikes  in  a 
train  of  unit  discharges  may  be  as  brief  as  i  m.sec.  or 
less,  implying  that  the  refractory  period  of  single 
neurons  following  a  discharge  is  substantially  shorter 
than  that  of  the  potential  recorded  from  the  ag- 
gregates of  neurons.   The  reason  for   this  difference 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


309 


between  single  neurons  and  neuronal  aggregates  is  not 
entirely  understood.  Perhaps  in  the  case  of  neuronal 
aggregates  the  processes  of  postexcitatory  depression 
have  superseded  the  phase  of  true  refractoriness  and 
therefore  make  it  appear  that  the  refractory  period  is 
prolonged.  The  true  refractory  period  is  believed  to 
be  the  same  as  the  period  during  which  the  repolariza- 
tion process  of  the  membrane  potential  of  the  active 
neurons  is  taking  place.  Its  value  is  determined  by 
the  rate  of  the  repolarization  process. 

Poitexcitatury  Di'pressiun 

The  phase  of  postexcitatory  depression  may  be 
defined  as  the  period  immediately  following  the  initial 
recovery  from  refractoriness  during  which  the  nervous 
tissue  undergoes  various  degrees  of  lowered  excitabil- 
ity. The  basic  pattern  of  the  event  exists  in  the  ex- 
citability cycle  of  any  kind  of  nervous  tissue  including 
the  cerebral  cortex,  the  spinal  cord,  peripheral 
nerves  and  sympathetic  ganglia. 

The  degree  and  duration  of  the  postexcitatory  de- 
pression vary  more  or  less  proportionately  with  the 
stimulus  strength  and  the  number  of  neurons  pre- 
viously activated  by  the  conditioning  stimulus.  There 
may  be  no  obvious  secondary  depression  following  the 
refractory  period  if  the  previous  response  is  weak. 
After  an  intense  response  of  the  cortex  the  postexcita- 
tory depression  may  last  for  several  hundred  milli- 
seconds. Extremely  severe  depression  lasting  for  a 
second  or  more  has  been  observed  following  the 
respon.se  of  the  cortex  treated  with  strychnine.  This  is 
true  for  cortical  responses  to  topical  stimulation  as 
well  as  for  those  to  afferent  impulses  (15).  By  u.sing 
paired  electric  shocks  applied  to  the  optic  nerve,  both 
Marshall  (53)  and  Clare  &  Bishop  (26)  demonstrated 
the  existence,  in  the  visual  cortex  and  in  the  lateral 
geniculate  body,  of  a  typical  excital:)ility  cycle  with  the 
supernormal  phase  followed  by  a  long  period  of  post- 
excitatory depression.  When  the  retina  is  continuously 
illuminated,  not  only  the  onset  but  also  the  cessation 
of  the  photic  stimulus  produce  the  phenomenon  of 
postexcitatory  depression  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  The 
visual  cortex  undergoes  a  period  of  severe  depression 
immediately  after  the  recovery  of  the  cortex  from  the 
refractoriness  caused  by  retina  excitation.  In  extreme 
cases  this  period  of  temporary  depression  may  last  for 
as  long  as  several  seconds.  A  similar  phenomenon  is 
present  in  the  auditory  system.  Rosenblith  el  al.  (61) 
observed  that  the  neural  respon.se  to  a  click  as  re- 
corded from  the  round  window  and  from  the  auditory 
cortex  of  the  cat  were  depressed  within  the  first  40  sec. 


after  sudden  exposure  to  continuous  tones.  The  post- 
excitatory inactivation  of  the  cortex  discus.sed  here 
may  constitute  a  physiological  basis  for  the  temporary 
blindness  and  deafness  following  a  sudden  exposure  to 
strong  light  and  loud  sound.  The  diininished  excitabil- 
ity of  neurons  during  the  period  of  postexcitatory 
depression  is  l)elieved  to  be  associated  with  the  mem- 
brane potential  changes  of  the  neuron  following  the 
discharge.  From  recent  microelectrode  studies  of  the 
electrical  properties  of  single  neurons  it  has  been 
observed  that  the  repolarization  of  the  membrane 
potential  may  develop  into  a  phase  of  hyperpolariza- 
tion  which  reaches  a  maximum  at  5  to  10  msec,  and 
may  last  for  as  long  as  100  msec.  During  the  period  of 
hyperpolarization  the  action  potential  of  the  neuron 
is  inhibited  (27,  28).  The  time  courses  of  the  repolari- 
zation and  the  hyperpolarization  processes  bear  a 
close  relationship  to  the  refractoriness  and  the  post- 
excitatory depression  of  the  neuron.  ' 

Pinodic  \  nnatuin  in  Cortical  Excitability 

Unique  to  the  sensory  area,  the  e.xcitability  state  of 
the  cortex  does  not  always  return  to  the  normal  level 
following  the  completion  of  a  usual  excitability  cycle 
but  undergoes  a  further  cyclic  waxing  and  waning 
with  regular  intervals.  The  periodic  excitability 
change  of  the  visual  cortex  was  described  bv  Bishop  in 
'933  Co)-  However,  he  believed  it  was  an  indication  of 
the  excitability  change  of  the  optic  pathways  rather 
than  of  the  cortical  neurons  them.selves.  The  periodic 
\  ariation  in  excitability  of  the  auditory  cortex  beyond 
the  unresponsive  period  caused  by  a  sound  stimulus 
was  obser\-ed  by  Jarcho  (38).  He  noticed  the  periodic 
depression  of  the  cortex  at  a  frequency  coincidental 
with  the  repetitive  corticothalamic  after-discharges. 
No  increased  excitability  was  seen  at  any  time,  how- 
ever. Jarcho's  finding  was  soon  confirmed  with  the 
further  disclosure  that  in  company  with  the  rising  and 
falling  of  the  corticothalamic  reverberating  waves, 
there  are  concomitant  increase  and  decrease  of  cortical 
excitability  (14,  15).  The  temporal  relation  between 
the  repetitive  waves  and  the  excitability  change  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  resulting  from  the  corticothalamic 
reverberating  activity  was  found  to  be  such  that  the 
cortical  excitability  is  increased  during  the  developing 
phase  of  the  re\-erberating  wave  and  decreased  during 
the  returning  phase  of  the  reverberating  wave.  An 
alignment  of  the  excitability  curve  and  the  contour  of 
the  reverberating  waves  on  the  same  time  scale  show 
that  the  maximum  of  the  increased  excitability  is 
reached  in  the  middle  of  the  developing  phase,  that 


310 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  maximum  of  decreased  excitability  is  reached  in 
the  middle  of  the  returning  phase  and  that  the  ex- 
citability remains  unchanged  both  at  the  peak  and  at 
the  valley  of  the  reverberating  waves.  Mathematically 
speaking,  the  sinusoidal  curves  representing  the 
reverberating  waves  and  the  excitability  changes  are 
90°  out  of  phase,  the  maximum  of  cortical  excitability 
being  one  quarter  of  a  period  ahead  of  the  peak  of  the 
reverberating  waves  (15). 

Though  intimately  related  with  the  corticothalamic 
re\-erberating  wa\es,  the  periodic  \ariation  of  cortical 
excitability  following  an  afTerent  stimulation  may  be 
manifested  in  the  absence  of  detectai^le  repetitive 
waves.  At  the  onset  of  continuous  illumination  of  the 
retina,  for  instance,  the  waxing  and  waning  of  the 
cortical  excitability  can  be  demonstrated  even  within 
the  prolonged  period  of  postexcitatory  depression  dur- 
ing which  the  reverberating  waves  are  not  distinctly 
visible  (16).  The  significance  of  the  periodic  excita- 
bility change  of  this  kind  is  not  known.  Its  relation 
with  the  spontaneous  brain  waves  has  been  discussed 
by  Gastaut  (37)  and  Lindsley  (44).  The  periodic 
variation  in  excitability  of  spinal  neurons  has  been 
described  by  Bernhard  (7),  but  the  mechanism  in- 
volved in  this  case  is  believed  to  be  diflferent.  The  rela- 
tion of  the  reticular  formation  to  cortical  excitability 
has  been  studied  by  King  et  al.  (40). 

Inlnaction  of  Afferent  Imjnilies  in  the  Cerebral  Cortex 

Information  concerning  the  excitabilit\  change  of 
the  cortex  on  which  the  present  discussion  is  based  is 
obtained  mainly  from  experiments  in  which  the  corti- 
cal excitability  is  determined  by  a  testing  volley 
having  the  same  source  as  the  conditioning  one.  When 
two  afferent  volleys  of  different  origin,  an  auditory 
and  a  callosal,  for  instance,  are  sent  to  the  same  locus 
of  the  cortex,  the  e\oked  responses  to  the  combined 
stimuli  are  characteristically  different.  The  cause  of 
the  difference  seems  to  lie  in  the  fact  that  the  callosal 
and  thalamic  afferent  volleys  arri\e  at  diflerent  strata 
of  the  cortex  and  activate  different  sets  of  neurons, 
among  which  a  certain  nmnber  of  common  elements 
are  involved  in  the  responses  to  both  \-olleys  (19). 
Probaljly  for  the  same  reason  the  cortical  response  to 
acoustic  stimuli  can  ije  inhibited  or  facilitated  by 
simultaneous  or  successi\e  stimulation  of  a  sxmmetri- 
cal  point  on  the  opposite  cortex  (10). 

In  an  extensive  study  of  the  interaction  at  different 
levels  of  the  variously  evoked  afferent  impulses, 
Amassian  (i)  observed  that  both  the  cortical  and  the 
thalamic   responses   to  the  second   of  two  successive 


stimuli,  delivered  at  certain  intervals  to  the  same  ner\e 
or  to  two  separate  branches  of  a  peripheral  nerve, 
were  always  defective.  The  blocking  of  the  cortical  or 
subcortical  response  by  an  antecedent  volley  was 
interpreted  as  inhibition  as  distinguished  from  oc- 
clusion. Occlusion,  by  definition,  denotes  the  phe- 
nomenon in  which  the  total  effect  of  two  processes 
when  activated  simultaneously  is  smaller  than  when 
activated  separately  due  to  partial  overlapping  or 
sharing  of  common  elements.  In  real  occlusion  one 
process  should  not  be  completely  abolished,  though 
it  may  be  greatly  lessened,  by  another  simultaneously 
occurring  process.  If  it  is,  it  would  merely  indicate 
that  the  common  elements  can  be  totally  activated  by 
either  of  the  two  processes.  Thus,  the  complete  block- 
ing of  the  cortical  or  subcortical  response  to  stimula- 
tion of  the  \olar  branch  of  the  ulnar  nerve  by  previous 
stimulation  of  the  dorsal  branch  of  the  same  nerve 
described  by  Amassian  may  be  regarded  as  a  phe- 
nomenon of  inhibition,  and  the  defect  of  cortical 
response  to  splanchnic  stimulation  preceded  by  a 
shock  to  the  tibial  ner\e  a  phenomenon  of  occlusion, 
since  the  splanchnic  nerve  and  the  tibial  nerve  do  have 
separate  focal  areas  of  projection  with  overlapping 
fringes  in  the  thalamus  and  the  cortex. 

This  argument  about  occlusion  is  perhaps  also 
applicable  to  Marshall's  investigation  of  the  inter- 
action between  the  ipsilateral  and  contralateral  visual 
pathways  which  converge  with  overlapping  terminal 
branches  on  some  common  neurons  in  the  visual 
cortex  (53).  It  is  significant  that  bilateral  interaction 
does  not  occur  at  the  geniculate  level,  since  the  optic 
fibers  from  the  two  retinae  do  not  mingle  but  termi- 
nate in  separate  laminae  of  the  lateral  geniculate 
bodv.  There  are  no  common  neurons  available  at  that 
level  for  the  impulses  from  both  sides  to  act  on  each 
other,  although  the  impulses  reach  the  same  nucleus. 
However,  if  two  stimuli  of  equal  strength  are  applied 
to  the  same  optic  nerve  at  proper  intervals  the  genicu- 
late response  to  the  second  stimulus  is  greater  than 
that  to  one  stimulus  alone.  Apparently,  the  neurons 
in  the  subliminal  fringe  are  recruited  into  action  due 
to  temporal  sunnnation. 

Modifiealion  of  Cortical  Excitability  by  Cointant 
Inflow  of  Afferent  Impulses 

It  has  long  been  known  that  the  proper  excitability 
level  in  an  animal  when  awake  is  maintained  by  an 
incessant  action  of  afferent  impulses.  It  has  frequently 
been  reported  clinically  that  patients  completely 
depri\ed  of  sensory  al)ilities  fall  asleep  immediately 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


311 


and  can  be  aroused  only  by  stimulation  of  some  sense 
organs.  Experimental  evidence  shows  that  afferent 
impulses  necessary  for  keeping  the  cerebral  cortex  in  a 
state  of  vigilance  reach  it  from  the  reticular  formation. 
Impulses  in  the  main  ascending  sensory  pathways  ap- 
parently travel  in  collaterals  entering  the  reticular 
formation,  since  they  cannot  maintain  wakefulness 
after  destruction  of  the  reticular  formation  (45,  63, 
64). 

Perhaps  one  of  the  more  illustrative  examples  show- 
ing the  effect  of  constant  afferent  inflow  on  cortical 
excitability  is  the  phenomenon  of  photic  potentiation 
(16).  The  electrical  response  to  stimulation  of  the 
lateral  geniculate  body  was  found  to  be  manv  times 
greater  when  the  retina  was  illuminated  than  when 
it  was  in  the  dark.  Not  only  was  the  size  of  the  cortical 
response  to  geniculate  stimulation  greater  but  also  the 
threshold  of  the  response  was  lower  during  retinal 
illumination  than  in  the  dark.  The  effect  of  photic 
potentiation  on  cortical  responses  dc\elopcd  pro- 
gressively after  the  onset  of  retinal  illumination  and 
reached  its  maximum  in  about  5  sec.  Once  the  maxi- 
mal effect  was  attained,  it  was  sustained  at  that  level 
as  long  as  the  retinal  illumination  persisted.  The 
enhanced  cortical  response  was  promptly  reduced  to 
the  preillumination  magnitude  as  soon  as  the  light 
stimulus  was  withdrawn.  The  mechanism  underhing 
this  potentiation  phenomenon  is  believed  to  lie  mainly 
in  the  lateral  geniculate  body.  As  is  well  known,  some 
retinal  elements  discharge  steadilv  at  low  frecjuencv'  in 
the  absence  of  light  which  is  apparently  not  strong 
enough  to  set  up  a  discharge  of  the  postsynaptic  neu- 
rons in  the  lateral  geniculate  body.  Nevertheless,  the 
summated  effect  of  the  incessant  bombardment  of 
these  subliminal  impulses  will  raise  the  excitability 
of  the  geniculate  neurons.  If  meanwhile  an  electric 
shock  is  applied  directly  to  the  geniculate  Ijody,  the 
neurons  which  are  normally  in  the  subliminal  fringe 
will  discharge  because  of  the  summation  of  the  existing 
presynaptic  impulses  induced  by  retinal  illumination 
and  the  electric  stimulus  directly  applied  to  the 
geniculate  body.  The  gradual  development  of  the 
photic  potentiation  effect  in  the  case  of  repetitive 
stimulation  of  the  geniculate  body  seems  to  constitute 
a  good  example  of  recruitment  in  the  truly  physio- 
logical sense  of  the  term. 

A  similar  process  is  present,  though  much  less  pro- 
nounced, in  the  auditory  system.  Rosenblith  et  al. 
(61)  described  the  enhancement,  by  exposure  to 
continuous  tones,  of  potentials  evoked  in  the  cortex  by 
a  click.  Their  results  suggest  that  tones  of  certain 
frequencies,  especially  tones  at  frequencies  between 


100  and  500  cycles  per  sec,  show  a  very  effective 
potentiation  effect  following  the  initial  period  of 
depression,  while  those  of  high  frequencies  are  not  as 
effective. 

Preliminary  observations  ha\e  been  reported  on  the 
potentiation  effect  of  continuous  retinal  illumination 
on  the  cortical  response  to  stimulation  of  the  auditory 
pathway  C'6).  The  interaction  between  the  visual  and 
auditory  impulses  appears  to  take  place  not  only  in 
the  cerebral  cortex  through  the  association  neurons 
but  also  in  subcortical  structures,  since  the  removal  of 
the  visual  cortex  cannot  completely  abolish  the 
photic  potentiation  effect  on  the  auditory  response. 
The  explanation  tor  the  interaction  between  the 
visual  and  auditory  impulses  at  the  subcortical  level 
is  rendered  diflicult  by  the  lack  of  direct  fiber  connec- 
tions between  the  medial  and  the  lateral  geniculate 
ijodies.  The  pulvinar  is  considered  a  possible  site  of 
subcortical  correlation  for  the  two  great  special  sen- 
.sory  systems.  Since  the  importance  of  the  core  struc- 
tures of  the  brain  stem  in  the  actixation  of  the  cerebral 
cortex  has  been  recognized  (50,  51,  56),  it  is  pos.sible 
that  the  interaction  at  the  cortical  level  between  the 
afferent  impulses  from  different  .sources  mav  be  exe- 
cuted indirectly  through  the  system  of  the  diencephalic 
and  mesencephalic  reticular  formation  where  collat- 
erals of  ascending  fibers  from  different  kinds  of  sense 
organs  converge.  The  centrally  located  area  in  the 
brain  stem  comprising  the  midbrain's  tegmentum,  the 
subthalamus,  the  hypothalamus  and  the  intralaminar 
portion  of  the  thalamus,  has  been  found  to  be  able  to 
desynchronize  the  electrocortical  activity  when  it  is 
stimulated  repetitively  at  a  rapid  rate.  It  can  also 
exert  various  effects  on  the  cortical  potentials  evoked 
by  single  shock  stimulation  of  a  particular  sensorv 
system  when  it  is  stimulated  singly  in  close  approxi- 
mation to  the  eliciting  shock.  Stimulation  of  this  area 
in  the  brainstem  has  also  ijeen  described  as  having  a 
catholic  activating  effect  on  the  entire  cerebral  cortex 
and  especially  the  frontal  lobe  of  the  brain.  This 
activating  system  receives  afferent  impulses  from 
various  sensory  sources  through  the  collaterals  of 
ascending  tract  fibers  and  transmits  them  to  the 
cerebral  cortex,  probably  both  by  the  thalamic  and 
the  capsular  corticipetal  routes. 


SUMM.'^RY 

An  evoked  potential  may  be  defined  as  the  detect- 
able electrical  change  in  the  brain  in  response  to  de- 
liberate stimulation  of  any  part  of  the  nervous  system. 


312 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV   -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Such  potentials  differ  from  the  spontaneous  electrical 
activity  in  that  they  have  a  definite  temporal  relation- 
ship to  the  onset  of  the  stimulus,  a  constant  pattern  of 
response  and  a  focus  of  maximal  response  in  the  brain. 
The  technique  of  evoked  potential  registration  has 
been  widely  used  as  a  tool  for  anatomical  studies  of 
the  central  nervous  system.  However,  the  conclusions 
drawn  from  the  results  of  such  studies  are  justified 
only  when  the  limitations  of  this  technique  are  duly 
considered. 

The  primary  response  of  the  evoked  cortical  poten- 
tial consists  of  a  presynaptic  component  produced  by 
the  impulses  from  the  afferent  fibers  and  a  post- 
synaptic component  produced  by  the  discharge  of 
intracortical  neurons.  These  two  components  can  be 
differentiated  from  each  other  by  various  experimental 
procedures,  such  as  by  study  of  a)  latency,  A)  the 
effect  of  repetitive  stimulation,  <)  the  relative  tolerance 
to  changes  in  internal  and  external  milieu  and  above 
all  d)  the  anatomical  considerations. 

The  neural  mechanism  for  elaboration  of  the 
evoked  cortical  potential  is  formulated  on  the  basis  of 
the  histological  organization  of  the  cerebral  cortex 
and  the  general  principles  of  neurophysiology.  In  a 
proposed  scheme  it  is  suggested  that  the  afferent  im- 
pulses from  the  thalamocortical  fibers  first  excite  the 
Golgi  type  II  cells  in  the  fourth  cortical  stratum  which 
in  turn  transmit  the  postsynaptic  impulses  to  star 
pyramids  and  the  star  cells  in  the  same  layer,  then  to 
small  and  medium  pyramidal  neurons  in  the  supra- 
granular  layers  and  finally  to  the  large  pyramidal 
neurons  in  the  deep  layers.  The  surface-positive  deflec- 
tion of  the  evoked  potential  is  attributed  to  the 
synchronized  propagation  of  impulses  along  the 
apical  dendrites  from  the  cell  body  of  pyramidal 
neurons  inward  to  the  cortical  surface.  The  depolar- 
ization process  of  the  cortical  pyramidal  neurons  is 
believed  to  start  always  at  the  cell  body  due  to  the 
effective  excitatory  action  of  the  pericorpu.scular 
synapses.  The  subliminal  excitation  of  the  paraden- 
dritic  synapses  has  the  effect  onl\  of  modifying  the 
excitability  state  of  the  neur(jn.   This  hypothesis  is 


supported  by  the  results  of  microelectrode  findings  of 
single  neurons. 

After-discharges  can  be  classified  into  three  kinds: 
/)  the  self-sustained  repetitive  firing  of  single  elements, 
2)  persistent  local  after-discharges  involving  the 
activity  of  closely  situated  intrinsic  neurons  and  3) 
the  periodic  after-discharges  involving  the  activity 
of  reverberating  circuits  interconnecting  distant 
structures.  Of  these  three,  the  most  frequently  ob- 
served in  the  central  nervous  system  is  the  local  after- 
discharge,  maintained  by  a  mechanism  of  self-re- 
excitation  through  collaterals  and  numerous  closed 
neuronal  circuits  within  the  cortex.  The  activities  of 
the  long  reverberating  circuit  are  not  to  be  confused 
with  other  kinds  of  periodic  waves  which  may  happen 
to  have  similar  frequency  and  similar  wave  form. 

The  ijrain  undergoes  a  cycle  of  excitability  changes 
accompanying  and  following  the  evoked  potential. 
The  true  refractory  period  which  lasts  for  less  than  a 
millisecond  is  thought  to  be  related  to  the  repolariza- 
tion process  of  the  neuron.  The  postexcitatory  de- 
pression which  may  last  for  as  long  as  100  msec,  is 
proi:)ably  a  functional  manifestation  of  the  hyper- 
polarization  process.  Because  of  the  supersession  of 
the  rcfraciorv  phase  by  the  process  of  postexcitatory 
depression,  the  value  of  the  true  refractory  period  of 
the  neuronal  aggregate  cannot  be  accurately  deter- 
mined. In  addition  to  the  regular  cxcitabilit\-  c\cle 
there  is  a  periodic  variation  of  excitability  accom- 
panying the  reverberating  activity  of  the  sensory 
cortex. 

Although  the  ev'oked  potentials  in  different  systems 
are  independent  processes,  they  do  show  interaction 
proi^ably  due  to  the  overlapping  of  their  fiber  distribu- 
tion or  the  con\crgence  of  the  afferent  impulses  on  the 
common  neurons,  or  through  the  integration  in  a 
general  activating  system  such  as  the  reticular  forma- 
tion. Such  interaction  of  afferent  impulses  in  the 
cerebral  cortex  makes  it  possible  for  the  constant 
afferent  inflow  in  any  particular  sensory  system  to 
modif\  the  lc\ei  of  cortical  excitability  as  a  whole. 


REFERENCE. S 


1.  Amassian,  V.  E.  .-1.  Rei.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  371, 

■952- 

2.  Amassian,  V.  E.  and  J.  L.  Devito.  Fer/.  Proc.  13:3,  1954- 

3.  Amassian,  V.  E.  and  R.  V.  Devito.  J.  .Neurophysiol .  17;  575, 

1954- 

4.  Barron,  D.  H.   and  B.  H.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Phy.uul.  92: 

276,  1938. 


5.  Bartlev,  ,S.  H.  and  G.  H.  Bishop.  Am.  J.  Plmio!.  103    159, 

'933- 

6.  Beecher,  H.  K.,  F.  K.  McDonougii  and  .\.  Forbes.  J. 

Neurophysiol.  i:  324,  1938. 

7.  Bernhard,  C.  G.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  7:  397,  1944. 

8.  Bishop,  P.  O.  and  J.  G.  McLeod.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  17:  387, 
'954- 


THE    EVOKED    POTENTIALS 


3'3 


g.    Bremer,  F.  /(''  Congr.  Neurol.  Intnmil.,  Rapp.  i  ■  7,  1949. 

10.  Bremer,  F.  Rev.  neural.  87:  162,  1952. 

1 1.  Brock,  L.  G.,  J.  S.  Coombs  and  J.  C.  Eccles.  J.  Physiol. 

'■7:  431.  1952- 

12.  Burns,  D.  B.  J.  Physiol.  112,  156,  1951- 

13.  Burns,  D.  B.  J.  Physiol.  127:  168,  1955. 

14.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  13:  236,  1950. 

15.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  14:  95,  1951. 

16.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  5,  1952. 

17.  Chang,  H.-T.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Qiiant.  Biol.  17.  189, 

IMS- 

18.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  16:  117,  1953. 

19.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  16:  133,  1953. 

20.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  16:  221,  1953. 

21.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18:  332,  1955. 

22.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18:  452,  1955. 

23.  Chang,  H.-T.  In:  Problems  of  the  Modern  Physiology  of  the 
Nervous  and  Muscle  Systems.  Tiflis:  Publ.  House,  .Acad,  of 
Sciences,  Georgian  SSR,  1956. 

24.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  19:  224,  1956. 

25.  Chang,  H.-T.  and  B.  Kaada.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  13:  305,  1950. 

26.  Clare,  M.  H.  and  G.  H.  Bishop.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  4:  311,  1952. 

27.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.   130: 

29'.  1955- 

28.  Coombs,  J.  S.,  J.  C.  Eccles  and  P.  Fatt.  J.  Physiol.  130; 
3^6,  1955. 

29.  Derbyshire,  A.  J.,  B.  Rempel,  A.  Forbes  and  E.  F. 
Lambert.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  116:  577,  1936. 

30.  Eyzaguirre,  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

87.  1955- 

31.  Eyzaguirre,  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J.  Ge>i.  Physiol.  39: 
120,  1955. 

32.  Eyzaguirre,  C.  and  S.  VV.  Kuffler.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

■55.  1955- 

33.  Forbes,  A.  and  B.  R.  Morison.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:112,  1939. 

34.  Forbes,  A.,  A.  F.  Battista,  P.  O.  Chatfield  and  J.  P. 
Garcia.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  i  :  141,  1949. 

35.  Frank,  K.  and  M.   G.   F.   Fuortes.  J.  Physiol.    130:  625, 

'955- 

36.  Galambos,  R.,  J.  E.  Rose,  R.  B.  Bromiley  and  J.  R. 
Hughes.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  359,  1952. 

37.  Gastaut,  H.,  Y.  Gastaut,  A.  Roger,  J.  Carriol  and  R. 

Naquet.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  3:  401,  1951. 

38.  Jarcho,  L.  W.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   12:  447,   1949. 

39.  Jasper,  H.  and  J.  Droogleever-Fortuyn.  .4.  Res.  Nerv. 
&  Merit.  Dis.,  Proc.  26:  272,  1947. 

40.  King,  E.,  R.  Naquet  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  J.  Pharmacol.  & 
Exper.   Therap.   119:  48,   1957. 


41.  KoKETSu,  K.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  184:  338,  1956. 

42.  Kristinnsen,  K.  and  G.  Courtois.  Eleclroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  i :  265,  1 949. 

43.  Li,  C.  L.  and  H.  Jasper.  J.  Physiol.  121 :  117,  1953. 

44.  Lindsle-^-,   D.    B.   Eleclroencephalog.  &   Clin.   .Neurophysiol.   4: 

443.  1952- 

45.  Lindsley,  D.  B.,  L.  H.  Schreiner,  VV.  B.  Knowles  and 
H.   \V.   Magoun.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol., 2: 

483.  1950- 

46.  Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  and  A.  K.  McIntvre.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  32: 

409,  1949- 

47.  LoRENTE  de  No,  R.  .4  Study  of  .Nerve  Physiology.  New  York: 
Studies  from  The  Rockefeller  Institute  for  Medical  Re- 
search 131-132,  1947. 

48.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  J.  Cell.  G?  Comp.  Physiol.  29:  207,  1947. 

49.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.  In:  The  Spinal  Cord,  edited  by  J.  L. 
Malcolm  and  J.  A.  B.  Gray.  Boston:  Little,  1953,  p.  132. 

50.  Magoun,  H.  W.  Physiol.  Rev.  30:  455,  1950. 

51.  Magoun,  H.  W.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  67:  145,   1952. 

52.  Marshall,  W'.  H.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  25,  1941. 

53.  Marshall,  VV.  H.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  12:  277,  1949. 

54.  Marshall,  VV.  H.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  2- 
177,  1950. 

55.  Marshall,  VV.,  C.  N.  VVoolse\  and  P.  Bard.  J.  .Neuro- 
physiol. 4 :  1 ,  1 94 1 . 

56.  MoRUZZi,  G.  AND  H.  W.  Magoun.  Eleclroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  1  :  455,  1949. 

57.  P.\TTON,  H.   D.   AND  V.   E.   Amassian.  J.  .Ncurophysiol.    17: 

345.  ■954- 

58.  Perl,  E.  R.  and  J.  U.  Casby.  J.  Neurophysiol.  17:  429,  1954. 

59.  Phillips,  C.  G.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  41 :  58,  1956. 

60.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hosp.  94:  238,  1954. 

61.  Rosenblith,  VV.  A.,  R.  Galambos  and  I.  J.  Hirsh. 
Science  1 1 1 :  569,  1950. 

62.  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  163:  746,   1950. 

63.  Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  W.  Taylor  and  H.  VV.  M.agoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  14:  461,  1951. 

64.  Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  W.  Taylor  and  H.  VV.  Magoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  14:  479,  1951. 

65.  Svaetichin,   G.   .Acta  physiol.   scandinav.   Suppl.   86,   24:  23, 

1951- 

66.  Tasaki,  I.,  E.  H.  Poi.ley  and  F.  Orrego.  J.  .Neurophysiol. 

17:454.  ■954- 

67.  Tunturi,  a.  R.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.  147:  311,  1946. 

68.  Woodbury,  J.  VV.  and  H.  D.  Patton.  Cold  Spring  Harbor 
Symp.  Qjiant.  Biol.   17:   185,  1952. 


CHAPTER    XIII 


Changes  associated  with  forebrain 
excitation  processes:  d.c.  potentials 
of  the  cerebral  cortex' 


JAMES  L.  O'LEARY 
SIDNEY  GOLDRING 


Divisions  of  Neurology  and  Neurosurgery,  and  the  Beaumont  May  Institute  of 
Neurology,  Washington  University  School  of  Medicine,  St.  Louis,  Missouri 


CHAPTER    CONTENTS 

Suggested  Technique  for  Cortical  D.C.  Recording 
Experimental  .Studies 

Spontaneous  SP  Changes  Correlated  with   Interruption   in 
Usual  Resting  Cortical  Rhythm 

After-effects  of  Evoked  Responses 

SP  Changes  Associated  with  Recruiting  Responses 

SP  After-effects  of  Strychnine  and  Veratrine  .Spikes 

SP  Concomitants  of  Convulsoid  Discharges 

D.C.  Changes  Which  Accompany  Spreading  Depression  (SD) 

Relation  of  Polarity  of  Evoked  Transient  to  Polarity  of  SP 
Change  Also  Consequent  to  Stimulation 

Effects  of  Stimulation  at  a  Distance  Along  a  Multisynaptic 
Path    Upon   Transcortical   SP 

Injury  Potential  Components 

Human  Studies  Using  Scalp  Recording 
Discussion  of  Origin  of  Steady  Potentials 
Summary 


BESIDES  THE  SPONTANEOUS  and  evoked  potentials  con- 
ventionally recorded  from  the  cerebral  cortex,  the 
exposed  brain  in  a  resting  state  ordinarily  shows  a 
voltage  difference  between  cortical  surface  and  ven- 
tricle (d.c.  potential).  If  the  brain  is  not  disturbed 
following  its  preparation  for  recording,  this  pia- 
ventricular  potential  may  remain  relativeh'  steady, 
and  for  that  reason  we  have  referred  to  it  as  steady 
potential  (SP). 

'  .'Mded  by  grants  from  the  .Allen  P.  and  Josephine  B.  Green 
Foundation  and  the  Public  Health  Service  (B-882). 


The  role  SP  plays  in  neuronal  functioning  can  be 
assayed  only  after  taking  account  of  the  nonneuronai 
sources  which  complicate  its  interpretation.  It  is  also 
necessary  to  differentiate  such  d.c.  potentials  from 
those  of  pH  and  oxygen  electrode  recording.  The 
latter,  although  employing  a  d.c.  method,  depend 
upon  the  change  which  occurs  in  a  critical  electrode 
as  a  result  of  a  tissue  change  in  its  milieu.  By  contrast, 
the  d.c.  recording  described  hereafter  employs  very 
stable  electrodes  to  register  changes  in  the  distribution 
of  electrical  charge  in  the  intervening  tissue. 

Reduced  oxygen  tension  incident  to  systemic  de- 
terioration, or  intracellular  poisoning  of  respiratory 
enzyme  systems  (e.g.  by  cyanide  ions)  can  produce 
predictable  changes  in  it.  Injury  effects  and  anesthesia 
are  unavoidable  complications  of  any  experimental 
neurophysiological  procedure  and  can  also  result  in 
SP  alterations.  Cbntrol  of  these  and  possibly  other 
factors  which  limit  the  applicability  of  the  method  is 
necessary  if  we  are  to  reach  an  understanding  of  the 
relation  between  spontaneous  and  evoked  potentials 
of  the  usual  electrocorticogram  and  SP  changes 
which  may  develop  coincidentally  or  subsequently. 

Several  early  studies  of  d.c.  potential  are  relevant 
to  the  approach  outlined  here.  Libet  &  Gerard  (30) 
first  showed  that  a  pia-\entricular  potential  exists  in 
the  frog  brain,  postulating  that  a  change  in  such 
potential  can  alter  spontaneous  cortical  activity. 
Jasper  &  Erickson  (22)  at  about  the  same  time  ob- 
served a  d.c.  voltage  component  associated  with  high 


315 


3i6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  -^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


voltage  convulsoid  discharge.  Later  Leao  (26,  27) 
proved  that  a  marked  d.c.  change  also  accompanies 
a  wave  of  spreading  depression  as  it  propagates  across 
the  cortex.  This  phenomenon  will  ije  discussed  in 
detail   under   experimental    findings. 

One  may  assume  with  Libet  &  Gerard  (30)  that  a 
significant  component  of  the  pia-\entricular  potential 
arises  from  an  end-to-end  polarization  of  the  cortical 
pyramids,  and  that  the  transcortical  potential  over  an 
area  of  corte.x  represents  chiefly  the  average  of  the 
polar  charges  of  the  contained  neurons.  However, 
the  potential  difference  along  an  individual  pyramidal 
neuron  has  relative  rather  than  absolute  pertinence, 
for  it  can  be  recorded  not  only  when  one  end  of  the 
cell  is  depolarized,  but  when,  for  any  reason,  the  two 
ends  become  unequally  depolarized.  The  polarization 
of  both  ends  might,  for  example,  be  lowered  un- 
equally, raised  unequally,  or  one  raised  and  one 
lowered.  With  externally  applied  polarization  as 
studied  by  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (2)  it  is  almost  certainly 
the  latter  which  occurs.  Surface-positixe  polarization 
applied  to  an  area  of  cortex  accentuates  the  surface- 
negative  component  of  evoked  potential  transients 
recorded  therefrom;  accentuation  of  the  surface- 
positive  component  occurs  during  applied  surface- 
negative  polarization. 

Reviewed  herein  are:  a)  the  requisites  for  reliable 
d.c.  recording  together  with  an  assay  of  difficulties 
which,  if  unrecognized,  may  render  experimental  data 
unreliable;  h}  the  transient  d.c.  alterations  which 
have  been  shown  to  accompany  or  follow  spontaneous 
or  induced  changes  in  the  pattern  of  the  usual  ECG, 
such  as  changes  in  spontaneous  activity,  evoked  re- 
sponses, recruiting  waves,  barbiturate  spindles,  strych- 
nine and  veratrine  spikes;  c)  changes  associated  with 
the  occurrence  of  high  voltage  convulsoid  activity; 
rf)  d.c.  change  accompanying  spreading  depression; 
i)  evidence  linking  the  polarity  of  usually  recorded 
evoked  potential  phenomena  with  those  of  accom- 
panying d.c.  change;  /)  d.c.  cortical  changes  pro- 
duced by  repetitive  stimulation  at  sites  distant  from 
the  recording  electrodes  and  requiring  transmission 
along  paths  containing  intervening  synapses;  and  g) 
injury  potential  effects. 


SUGGESTED    TECHNiqUE    FOR   CORTICAL   D.C.    RECORDING 

For  many  experimental  studies  it  is  important  to 
monitor  the  potential  continuously.  Minor  displace- 
ments of  the  recording  electrodes  or  injury  (either  of 
which  mav  ije  due  to  movement  of  the  animal),  an 


obstruction  in  the  airway,  or  periodic  excesses  of 
stimulation,  can  unstabilize  a  preparation  temporarily 
or  permanently.  Then,  swings  of  several  millivolts 
from  one  polarity  to  the  other  may  occur.  Movement 
of  the  electrodes  alone  may  occasion  swings  in  po- 
tential in  either  direction,  whereas  with  the  other 
conditions  mentioned  the  changes  are  characteristic; 
these  will  be  discussed  later.  In  the  rabbit  (less  often 
in  our  experience  in  the  cat)  SP  swings  occur  which 
accompany  either  spreading  depression  (Leao)  or 
the  appearance  of  high  voltage  convulsoid  activity. 
Li  the  rabbit  the  latter  is  a  common  enough  accom- 
paniment of  excesses  of  electrical  stimulation. 
L  nidirectional  drifts  may  also  occur  during  systemic 
deterioration,  or  with  oxygen  lack  or  deepening 
anesthesia.  These  necessitate  quick  recognition  if  the 
preparation  is  to  continue  to  provide  reliable  data. 

To  measure  SP  one  needs  nonpolarizable  elec- 
trodes. Continuous  monitoring  during  experiments 
lasting  for  se\eral  hours  indicates  the  need  for  stable 
electrodes.  We  have  used  calomel  half-cells  having  a 
difference  of  potential  in  Tyrode's  of  0.5  to  i.o  mv.  A 
flexible  pipette  may  be  led  from  one  member  of  a 
pair  to  the  cortical  surface  of  the  selected  region; 
that  from  the  other  can  be  introduced  either  into  the 
\entricle  or  the  subcortical  white  matter.  In  the 
rabbit  the  former  (ventricular)  position  to  the  surface 
gives  an  individual  diff'erence  of  potential  of  surface- 
positive  polarity  amounting  to  i  to  4  mv.  If  the  deep 
electrode  tip  is  introduced  into  the  subcortical  white 
matter  the  potential  difference  may  be  larger  due  to 
the  addition  of  injury  potential.  Monopolar  recording 
from  surface  cortex  to  a  point  upon  the  periosteum 
gi\es  a  change  of  the  same  polarity  as  that  evident 
with  transcortical  recording.  The  diff"erence  of  po- 
tential is  greater  than  the  pia-\entricular  potential 
ijut  less  than  that  led  between  surface  and  white 
matter.  Transcortical  (or  pia-ventricular)  d.c.  re- 
cording is  ijclieved  to  off'er  greater  promise  for  estab- 
lishing correlates  with  spontaneous  and  evoked  ac- 
tivity of  the  usual  ECG  because  of  the  more  localized 
leading. 

A  conventional  condensor-coupled  electroencepha- 
lograph as  well  as  a  d.c.  amplifier  ma\-  be  used  both 
for  monitoring  and  for  the  study  of  d.c.  changes.  It 
is  only  necessary  to  short-circuit  the  input  4  to  8 
times  per  sec.  Each  short  circuit  discharges  such 
potentials  as  have  accumulated  upon  the  input  con- 
densors  because  their  time  course  has  been  too  long 
to  permit  passage  into  the  amplifier.  The  discharge 
causes  the  pens  to  return  momentarily  to  the  baseline 
of  the  amplifier.  When  a  chopper  is  u.sed,  if  the  record- 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED    WITH    FOREBRAIN    EXCITATION    PROCESSES 


3'7 


ing  apparatus  is  set  so  that  a  negative  ECG  transient 
registers  as  an  upward  deflection,  a  correlated  nega- 
tive SP  change  appears  as  a  series  of  down-going  pips, 
the  amplitude  of  which  determines  the  \oltage  of  the 
change.  Vice  versa,  positive  SP  changes  are  recorded 
as  up-going  pips.  The  deflection  of  the  pips  then  is 
opposite  to  the  direction  in  which  ECG  transients 
are  recorded,  although  each  has  the  same  polarity. 
The  reason  is  that  each  interruption  of  the  input 
returns  the  pens  from  a  positive  or  a  negatise  potential 
value  to  the  zero  baseline  of  the  amplifier  (fig.  i). 
The  conventional  ECG  can  be  recorded  upon  other 
channels  of  the  same  electroencephalograph,  using 
neighboring  pairs  of  polarizable  electrodes  tor  the 
pickup. 

The  recording  system  must  be  flexible  enough  to 
detect  microvolt  changes  at  the  same  time  that  it  is 
prepared  to  register  a  change  of  several  milli\olts.  To 
accomplish    this    two    devices    are   used:   o)   several 


channels  of  amplification  record  from  the  same  lead 
combination  at  different  sensitivities;  A)  a  balancing 
potentiometer  is  placed  in  series  with  one  of  the 
calomel  half-cells  to  oppose  the  electrical  effect  of  any 
sizeable  shift  through  the  application  of  a  counter- 
voltage.  The  amplitude  of  an  ,SP  change  can  then  be 
read  directly  from  the  potentiometer.  During  swings 
of  seseral  millivolts  the  more  sensitive  channels  are 
first  turned  ofl' while  the  least  sensitive  one  is  balanced. 
Thereafter,  the  others  are  balanced  in  order  toward 
the  most  sensitive  one.  That  one  is  used  continuously 
to  record  SP  concomitants  of  evoked  and  spontaneous 
activity,  and  the  d.c.  changes  it  records  are  the  ones 
which  can  he  correlated  most  directly  with  ECG 
manifestations  of  neuronal  activity.  From  the  calomel 
half-cells,  records  of  the  quicker  d.c.  changes  which 
are  registered  upon  the  most  sensitive  channel  of  the 
electroencephalograph  can  also  be  led  through  a  d.c. 
amplifier    to    a    cathode-ray    oscillograph.     During 


—     Mi 


,",VJ'.''.AAAM'\'^'" 


I  sec 


FIG.  I.  Steady  potential  change  accompanying  cortical  recruiting  in  the  rabbit.  Light  ether  anes- 
thesia. Stimulation  in  medial  thalamus.  Recording  from  frontal  cortex.  A.  Recorded  by  conven- 
tional condensor -coupled  electroencephalograph  the  input  of  which  is  short-circuited  8  times  per 
sec.  Negative  polarity  recruiting  responses  are  recorded  as  upward  deflections,  and  black  dots  indi- 
cate the  first  and  last  responses  of  a  series.  The  SP  change  is  recorded  as  a  series  of  downward 
deflections,  each  indicating  a  short  circuit  of  the  input.  Although  opposite  in  direction  from  the 
deflections  representing  the  recruiting  transients  each  represents  a  d.c.  change  of  negative  polarity. 
Records  B  and  C  serve  to  clarify  the  situation.  For  each  the  base  line  from  which  the  d.c.  change  is  a 
departure  is  indicated  by  a  straight  white  line.  B.  The  same  recruiting  response  recorded  upon  an 
oscilloscope  with  use  of  a  direct-coupled  amplifier.  Note  negative  steady  potential  change  accom- 
panying recruiting  series.  Surface  of  the  cortex  remained  negative  with  respect  to  the  underlying 
white  matter  for  approximately  750  msec,  following  the  last  recruiting  response.  C.  Another  re- 
cruting  response  series  recorded  under  similar  conditions  to  that  of  B.  Input  is  again  short-circuited 
8  times  per  sec.  as  in  A.  Note  that  each  short  circuit  returns  the  beam  to  the  base  line  of  the  amplifier. 
Had  this  series  been  recorded  by  means  of  the  same  amplifier  using  condensor  coupling,  the  d.c. 
shift  should  have  been  eliminated  and  the  oscilloscope  record  would  have  been  the  counterpart  of  .4. 
The  chopper  signals  on  C  have  been  retouched  due  to  printing  difficulties. 


3i8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY'    I 


19     20     21      Zl     23    2+ 


Time  in  Minutes 

FIG.  2.  Graph  of  change  in  SP  during  asphyxia  which  resuhed  from  clamping  airway.  /,  tracheal 
airway  clamped;  i',  ECG  completely  suppressed;  3,  heart  stops.  [From  Goldring  &  O'Leary  (11).] 


oscillographic  recording  upon  strips  of  film,  the 
chopper  is  temporarily  disconnected.  By  convention, 
shifts  in  potential  from  the  base  line  will  be  referred 
to  as  positive  or  negative  with  respect  to  the  surface 
electrode. 

Under  optimal  conditions  the  initial  determination 
of  pia-ventricular  SP  has  varied  usually  between  0.5 
and  5.0  mv  positive  (i  i).  These  data  include  meas- 
urements upon  20  rabbits  which  were  anesthetized 
with  ether  for  only  the  brief  tiine  required  to  pro- 
cainize  the  skin  and  paravertebral  muscles  over  the 
C5  cord  segment  and  divide  the  cord  there.  Anesthesia 
could  then  be  discontinued,  and  the  scalp  similarly 
infiltrated  with  procaine  as  a  prelude  to  exposing  the 
brain  for  recording.  With  insertion  of  the  deep  lead 
into  the  subcortical  white  matter  (in  the  cat),  negative 
injury  potential  about  the  deep  electrode  tip  con- 
tributes to  the  positivity  at  the  surface,  and  larger 
voltage  discrepancies  may  be  recorded.  In  human 
subjects  under  nitrous  oxide  and  thiopental  anesthesia 
in  whom  the  pia-ventricular  potential  has  been  re- 
corded, the  measurement  in  the  majority  of  cases  has 
been  0.3  to  0.5  mv  positive  (17).  Here  injury  potential 
was  not  believed  to  be  a  significant  complicating 
factor.  Returning  to  the  animal  experiments,  we  note 
that  SP  may  not  fluctuate  more  than  0.5  mv  during 
several  hours  of  continuous  recording  if  no  procedure 
is  undertaken  after  the  preparation  of  the  animal. 
With  the  high  cord  section  (at  Ci),  necessitating  arti- 
ficial respiration,  systemic  failure  has  often  developed 
in  rabbits;  under  this  circumstance  SP  may  show  a 
continuous  negative  drift  terminating  with  the  death 
of  the  animal.  Clamping  the  trachea  in  an  animal  in 


good  condition,  in  which  SP  has  not  undergone  sig- 
nificant alterations  during  several  hours  of  continuous 
recording,  will  result  m  a  significant  SP  change  (11). 
While  \ariaijle  and  somewhat  complex,  this  consists 
principally  of  an  initial  positive  shift  (2  mv)  followed 
by  an  even  more  prominent  negative  one  (4  mv). 
The  major  change  which  follows  clamping  of  the 
airway  usually  ends  in  8  min.  The  heart  ceases  to 
beat  between  the  maximum  and  the  end  of  the  nega- 
tive deflection  (fig.  2). 

Leao  (26)  demonstrated  a  similar  negative  shift 
incident  to  cortical  anemia  and  van  Harreveld  el  al. 
have  also  shown  a  negative  SP  shift  with  asphyxia 
(41,  43).  Other  investigators,  leading  from  the  cortex 
and  using  the  sciatic  nerve  as  reference  point,  have 
also  recorded  SP  shifts  with  anoxia  and  asphyxia 
(4,    10). 

The  injection  of  malononitrile,  which  liberates 
cyanide  ions,  evokes  an  analagous  picture  (18).  This 
has  been  followed  during  the  intravenous  injection 
of  between  10  and  20  ml  of  a  freshly  prepared  i  per 
cent  solution  delivered  by  Murphy  drip.  As  the  in- 
jection proceeds  the  animal  becomes  hyperpneic, 
and  at  aijout  that  time  the  ECG  commences  to  slow, 
with  the  appearance  of  random  components  showing 
higher  voltage  than  previously.  As  the  ECG  changes, 
SP  commences  to  shift  positively.  With  further  de- 
terioration in  the  preparation  the  ECG  becomes 
isoelectric,  and  SP  continues  to  shift  positively  to  a 
total  of  2.5  mv.  Clonic  convulsive  movements  may 
appear  during  the  positive  phase.  These  electrical 
events  can  be  reversed  if  5  per  cent  sodium  thiosulfate 
is  injected  at  the  beginning  of  the  positive  SP  change. 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED    WITH    FOREBRAIN    EXCITATION    PROCESSES 


319 


Then  the  ECG  and  SP  will  revert  to  the  preinjection 
status.  Unless  the  thiosulfate  is  injected  the  change 
continues  for  3  to  8  min.;  respiration  stops  and  shortly 
thereafter  the  heart  also.  Meanwhile  SP  undergoes  an 
opposite  shift  which  carries  it  3  to  4  mv  more  negative 
than  it  was  at  the  start  of  the  experiment  (fig.  3). 

We  ha\e  followed  .SP  under  ether  anesthesia  more 
carefully  than  under  any  other.  Commencing  at  a 
light  level  of  anesthesia  and  deepening  it  gradually, 
SP  may  remain  relatively  steady  although  occasionally 
a  positive  shift  may  occur.  If  inhalation  anesthesia 
is  carried  too  deep,  SP  may  shift  negatively  by  com- 
parison with  the  preceding  light  anesthesia  baseline. 
However,  as  long  as  anesthesia  is  maintained  at  a 
relatively  light  level,  the  changes  it  occasions  in  SP 
are  relatively  insignificant,  and  induced  physiological 
variations  are  much  like  those  encountered  in  a  rela- 
tively unanesthetized  state.  Finally,  solutions  added 
to  the  cortical  surface  which  are  not  isotonic  may 
disturb  SP  for  sexeral  minutes.  These  are  diffusion 
potential  artifacts. 

Summarizing,  one  must  emphasize  that  to  provide 
a  stable  base  for  measuring  induced  SP  changes  there 
should  be  careful  control  of  injury,  anesthesia  and 
oxygen  tension. 


EXPERIMENT.'\L  STUDIES 

Spontaneous   SP   Changes   Correlated  with   Interruption 
in  Usual  Resting  Cortical  Rhythm 

It  often  is  not  possible  to  demonstrate  SP  altera- 
tions which  coincide  with  a  change  in  the  ECG 
rhythm  of  the  lightly  anesthetized  animal.  However, 
in  the  rabbit  (12)  we  have  noted  repeated  brief  nega- 
tive shifts  amounting  to  0.6  mv  which  coincide  with 
low  voltage  ECG  intervals  between  runs  of  regular 
frequency  activity.  Furthermore,  a  relatively  rapid 
increase  in  ECG  amplitude  with  substitution  of 
slower  for  usual  frequencies  may  show  an  accom- 
panying positive  SP  shift.  In  the  pentobarbitalized 
cat  a  significant  negative  shift  can  be  recorded  during 
and  succeeding  barbiturate  spindles,  sometimes  out- 
lasting the  spindle  by  several  seconds. 

After-effects  of  Evoked  Responses 

Since  singly  evoked  responses  are  quick  transients, 
the  only  SP  change  which  could  be  expected  to  occur 
would  appear  as  an  aftermath.  We  have  studied  such 
after-effects  in  the  visual  cortex  of  the  rabbit  (11,  12) 
together  with  the  conditions  leading  to  their  summa- 


o 


7 
6 
5 
4 
3 
2 
/ 

0 
/ 
2 
3 
4 
5 


6 


+ 


T 


ime    in  minuxeb 


ici 


I    Z    3   4    5    6    7   S    9  10  1/  IZ  13  14  15  lb  17  18  19  10  2J  22  2i  24 


FIG.  3.  Graph  of  change  in  SP  during  intravenous  injection  of  20  ml  of  i  per  cent  solution  of 
malononitrile.  a,  start  of  injection;  b,  ECG  commences  to  become  isoelectric;  c,  respiration  stops. 


320 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tion  during  repetitive  stimulation.  The  after-effects 
of  singl\-  evoked  responses  may  not  be  evident  at  the 
beginning  of  an  experiment  but  appear  as  the  experi- 
ment proceeds.  They  may  be  positise,  negative  or 
diphasic  when  evoked  by  stimuli  applied  to  the  op- 
posite optic  nerve  or  the  corresponding  lateral  genicu- 
late nucleus.  The  positive  SP  component  follows  the 
evoked  response  transient  immediately  and  may 
reach  a  voltage  of  0.4  mv  and  persist  for  1.5  sec.  (fig. 
4).  In  diphasic  after-effects  the  positive  component 
is  usually  of  briefer  duration  (0.5  sec.)  and  is  followed 
by  a  negative  one  of  similar  voltage  lasting  for  1.5 
sec.  When  a  negative  after-effect  alone  is  encountered 
its  start  is  delayed  for  0.3  to  0.5  sec.  after  the  evoked 
respon.se  is  over. 

When  repetitive  stimulation  of  the  optic  nerve 
(submaximal  for  cortical  evoked  responses^  with  fre- 
quencies of  20  to  30  per  sec.  continued  for  2  to  10 
sec.  is  used,  negative  after-effects  are  oljserved  to  sum, 
and  positive  after-effect  is  minimal  (12,  fig.  5).  How- 
ever, a  positive  change  may  replace  the  negative  one 
if  cortical  excitabilitx'  had  been  significantly  changed 
by  a  preceding  major  paroxysm  (fig.  5F,  G).  Near- 
threshold  stimulation  of  the  lateral  geniculate  nucleus 
also  gives  chiefly  summation  of  negative  after-effect 
(12).  At  one  half  maximum  to  maximum  stimulus 
strength,  positivity  usually  develops  early  in  the 
course  of  stimulation,  to  he  replaced  later  by  a  nega- 
tive SP  change  which  persists  past  the  end  of  stimula- 
tion. From  this  and  other  evidence  we  have  concluded 
that  in  the  response  to  repetitive  stimulation,  positive 
and  negative  after-effects  sum  algeijraically,  different 
effects  predominating  at  different  strengths  of  stimu- 
lation. This  indication  of  the  existence  of  processes  of 
opposite  electrical  sign,  the  electrical  manifestations 
of  which  can  cancel  completeh',   may  relate  to  the 


-5^^~UAI--lA|^||J[A_w^^rv^f^ 


,  ECS        I 

I"         I  ! 


Asp 


V-v'-Jl^^-f..'^> 


A  ecg'  "^' 


-A/ — ._ 


B 


■.^.t^l^/^-^-fffff^l^iffftm^ 


FIG.  4.  .^fter-effects  which  follow  primary  visual  response  in 
rabbit.  After  each  evoked  response  there  is  a  diphasic  (positive- 
negative)  after-effect.  Black  dots  indicate  position  of  first  short- 
circuiting  signal  to  occur  after  each  response.  [From  Goldring 
&  O'Leary  (11).] 


B 


.-v^~^^.^^M/-^V•^ 


-'V/-v.T^v.^^iV<virnwTlffflfnt1t^^ 


/v'Tr-'^^r\|nYi^nT(TiTrmTfifMfrrmf^ 


'%..~'*"|i''-— -'TTtTfrnrn'irfrnTiYrror^v^YiYf-^^ 


/vl'^-v.^TiTnifnTfr'iTinnrmr'^^ 


— ^^^I'h^MT*.'*'^-*^  ■   ■»-  'M*  *~^»'  ^Jkt^<*  — w  "iNo^N  %^^^^iA»  ■  .1^  > 


FIG.  5.  SP  change  resulting  from  repetitive  stimulation  of 
the  optic  nerve  at  different  strengths.  A.  Diphasic  after-effect 
of  a  single  evoked  visual  response  for  comparison.  Stimulus 
frequency  for  subsequent  records  of  figure  was  25  per  sec. 
For  each  strip  the  initial  evoked  response  in  ECG  marks  the 
beginning  of  stimulation  and  the  arrow  its  cessation.  B.  Nega- 
tive SP  change  during  repetitive  stimulation  at  threshold. 
C.  Stimulus  strength  one-third  maximal;  here  negativity  is 
most  prominent.  I).  Stimulus  strength  half  ma.ximal;  the 
negativity  develops  somewhat  later.  E.  .Stimulus  strength 
maximal;  the  negativity  appears  later  and  is  not  as  maiked  as 
in  C  and  D.  Initial  positivity  in  SP  is  seen  to  occur  at  the  start 
of  stimulation.  F.  Similar  repetitive  stimulation  5  min.  following 
a  major  lasting  shift  in  SP;  stimulus  strength  as  in  C.  The  SP 
change  is  now  surface-positive.  G.  Same  as  F  except  that  stimu- 
lus strength  is  that  o{  E.  [From  Goldring  S"  O'Leary  (12).] 


ditiiculty  of  demonstrating  SP  after-effects  early  in  the 
course  of  some  experiments. 

SP  Changes  Associated  zvith  Recruiting  Responses 

With  repetitive  stimulation  (6  to  20  per  sec,  30  v, 
o. I  msec,  duration)  in  the  midline  thalamus  of  the 
rabbit  under  light  ether  anesthesia,  a  negative  cortical 
SP  change  amounting  to  0.2  to  0.6  inv  ordinarily 
develops  during  the  rise  in  amplitude  of  the  conven- 
tional ECG  transients  of  negative  polarity  as  shown 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED.  WITH    FOREBRAI.N    EXCITATION    PROCESSES 


321 


in  figure  i  (15,  16).  It  also  persists  significantly  after 
tiie  period  of  stimulation.  Oscillographic  recording 
shows  that  there  is  a  significant  SP  negative  change 
after  even  the  first  spike  of  a  series,  and  that  this 
change  is  summated  with  the  after-effects  of  succeed- 
ing higher  amplitude  spikes  as  the  recruiting  series 
continues.  The  persistence  of  the  SP  negati\ity  after 
the  stimulus  is  turned  off  indicates  that  it  is  truly  an 
after-effect  disturbance  comparable  to  that  which 
follows  single  evoked  responses.  Increasing  the 
stimulus  frequency  to  between  6  and  20  per  sec.  (the 
voltage  and  shock  duration  remaining  constant) 
increases  the  amplitude  and  duration  of  the  negative 
SP  change.  At  20  per  sec.  a  further  increase  in  ampli- 
tude and  duration  of  SP  negati\ely  occurs  if  the 
stimulus  duration  is  increased  from  o.i  to  i  .0  msec. 

SP  After-effects  of  Strychnine  and  Veratrine  Spikes 

The  effect  of  strychnine  has  been  studied  in  the 
rabbit  and  the  cat  (12,  13,  14).  In  the  rabbit  a  0.05 
per  cent  strychnine  solution  applied  to  the  cortex 
may  be  of  sufficient  strength  to  suppress  the  spon- 
taneous component  of  the  ECG;  a  minor  0.3  to 
0.4  mv  negative  SP  shift  appears  within  10  min. 
following  the  application  of  the  drug  to  the  cortical 
surface.  The  strychnine  spikes  which  occur  sporad- 
ically at  this  time  show  no  detectable  after-effect. 
However,  a  solution  sufficiently  strong  to  occasion 
intermittent  repetitive  spike  paroxysms  (0.5  per  cent) 
causes  a  positive  after-effect  to  appear  following  each 


random  spike  which  appears  between  the  paroxysms. 
With  application  of  a  crystal  of  strychnine,  negative 
after-effects  follow  each  instead.  A  o.  i  per  cent 
strychnine  sulfate  solution  applied  to  the  cortex  of 
the  cat  also  occasions  a  minor  negative  SP  shift,  and 
here  after-effects  of  strychnine  spikes  have  a  negative 
polarity.  When  the  single  spikes  occur  in  rapid  suc- 
cession the  negativities  associated  with  individual 
spikes  summate  (fig.  6).  The  SP  change  which  occurs 
during  a  strychnine  activated  paroxysm  will  be  re- 
ferred   to    later. 

X'eratrine  hydrochloride  (10^^)  applied  to  the 
cortex  of  the  pentobarbitalized  cat  unstabilizes  SP 
almost  immediately,  resulting  in  one  or  more  5  to 
1 5  mv  negative  shifts  from  each  of  which  SP  may 
recov-er  significantly  (13,  14).  A  plot  of  several  such 
shifts  following  \eratrine  application  shows  a  down- 
ward negati\e  drift,  and  the  SP  does  not  again  reach 
its  value  at  the  start  of  the  experiment.  Finally,  how- 
ever, it  stabilizes  upon  a  plateau.  At  that  time  the 
initial  positive  phase  of  the  evoked  response  becomes 
significantly  prolonged  and  spikes  of  dominantly 
positive  polarity  appear  spontaneously,  or  may  be 
initiated  (as  in  the  visual  cortex)  by  turning  the  room 
lights  on  or  off.  Each  evoked  response  and  each  such 
spike  is  accompanied  by  a  positive  after-effect  which 
may  endure  for  5  to  10  sec.  (fig.  7).  Barbiturate 
spindles  also  come  to  show  a  principally  positive 
polarity  in  the  ECG,  and  they,  too,  show  a  positive 
polaritv  SP  change  which  persists  significantly  after 
the  end  of  the  spindle  (13,  fig.  8).   If  strychnine  is 


.v«,^^W^Il'> 


Rg.   6 


Fig.   7 


,UH)to|W^MjVui/V^    A 


,wv%1 


.       '        '        I:200/iV 

I-  I      I  sec      1         ^ 


200/1 V-  1\__ 

I  sec 


ZOO  juW- 


FIG.  6.  Effect  of  0.1  per  cent  strychnine  sulfate  solution 
applied  to  cortical  surface  of  cat  under  pentobarbital  anes- 
thesia. /  and  2  indicate  ECG  and  SP  records,  respectively. 
A.  Individual  spontaneous  strychnine  spikes  with  negative  after- 
effects. B.  Occurrence  of  a  cluster  of  spontaneous  strychnine 
spikes  with  summation  of  the  negative  after-effects.  [From 
Goldring  &  O'Leary  (14).] 


Fic.  7.  Effect  of  application  of  veratrine  hydrochloride 
(io~')  to  surface  of  the  cortex  of  the  cat  under  pentobarbital 
anesthesia.  /  and  -'  indicate  ECG  and  .SP  records,  respectively. 
A.  Single  spontaneous  positive  veratrine  spike  with  a  long 
positive  after-effect.  B.  A  series  of  such  spikes  with  positive  after- 
effects. [From  Goldring  cl?  O'Leary  (13).] 


322  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


isec 


.,v<^^v^^W|fJ^A'W^^ 


FIG.  8.  SP  change  accompanying  barbiturate  spindles  in  a 
pentobarbitalized  cat.  /  and  2  indicate  ECG  and  SP  records, 
respectively.  .-1.  Spontaneous  barbiturate  spindles  with  tran- 
sients which  are  chiefly  negative  accompanied  by  a  negative 
shift  in  SP  which  outlasts  the  spindle.  B.  Same  cat  shortly  after 
the  application  of  veratrine  (io~0  'o  cortical  surface.  The 
transients  in  the  spindles  are  now  chiefly  positive  and  the  SP 
change  accompanying  the  spindle  has  also  become  positive. 
[From  Goldring  &  O'Leary  (13).] 


applied  before  veratrine  the  barbiturate  spindles 
may  at  first  show  an  accentuation  of  their  negative 
components  accompanied  by  nes;ati\e  after-effect; 
when  veratrine  takes  effect  the  principal  polarity  of 
the  spindle  changes  from  negative  to  positise  and  the 
after-effect  also  comes  to  have  a  positive  polarity. 

SP  Concomitants  of  Convtilsoid  Discharge 

These  have  been  studied  in  detail  in  the  rabbit 
(11,  12).  Such  a  cortical  discharge  can  be  initiated 
locally  by  surface-positive  polarization  across  the 
cortex,  by  repetitive  stimulation  (10  to  16  per  sec.) 
in  the  related  thalamic  relay  nucleus,  or  by  strychnine 
applied  locally  in  sufficient  concentration.  In  animals 
stimulated  repeatedly  and  vigorously  such  paroxysms 
may  commence  to  appear  intermittently  without 
preceding  activation.  The  latter  situation  is  analogous 
to  the  abrupt  appearance  of  con\ulsi\e  discharge  in 
man. 

With  few  exceptions  the  course  of  SP  change  during 
convulsoid  activity  has  been  a  positive  shift  of  i.o  to 
1.5  mv  correlated  with  the  build-up  of  the  discharge 
in  the  ECG  tracing.  As  the  tonic  discharge  becomes 
clonic  the  SP  commences  to  return  toward  the  pre- 
paroxysmal  balance;  as  the  discharge  ceases  SP  con- 
tinues to  shift  negatively,  sometimes  reaching  a  value 
of  5  mv  negative  to  the  preparoxysmal  balance  (fig. 
9).  When  paroxysms  follow  each  other  in  quick  suc- 
cession, a  positive  SP  shift  occurs  with  each,  and  a 
negative  one  coincides  with  the  intenseizure  silent 
period. 

The  initiation  of  a  paroxysm  by  polarization 
applied  acro.ss  the  cortex  from  surface  to  white  matter 
has   particular   interest   in   view   of  the   polarization 


[^Oj^KRSQQS^Q 


-  5oq//V 


z  100  msec 
pos=//p 


FIG.  9.  Steady  potential  change  during  paroxysm  in  rabbit. 
Light  ether  anesthesia.  Paro.xysm  induced  by  repetitive  10  per 
sec.  stimulation  of  the  lateral  geniculate  nucleus  with  bipolar 
electrodes.  Straight  white  line  of  each  trace  indicates  the  base 
line  of  the  ampliher  from  which  the  d.c.  change  is  a  departure. 
Record  from  optic  cortex  with  transcortical  leading,  positive 
up.  .'1.  Start  of  stimulation  of  lateral  geniculate  nucleus  indi- 
cated by  an  evoked  response  following  each  stimulus.  No  con- 
sistent change  in  SP  was  observed  during  this  period.  B.  1 2  sec. 
after  start  of  stimulation.  In  this  strip  the  positive  phase  of 
evoked  response  has  dropped  out  and  the  later  negative  com- 
ponent of  evoked  response  has  commenced  to  double,  indicating 
the  start  of  the  paro.xysm  which  persists  into  the  poststimulatory 
period.  Vertical  white  lines  indicate  end  of  period  of  stimula- 
tion. As  paroxysm  commences  SP  commences  to  shift  positively. 
C.  15  sec.  after  start  of  stimulation.  SP  has  continued  to  shift 
positively.  D.  20  sec.  after  the  start  of  stimulation.  Further 
positive  shift  as  the  poststimulatory  paroxysm  reaches  its  maxi- 
mum. E.  25  sec.  after  start  of  stimulation.  Paroxysm  diminishing 
in  intensity  and  SP  is  now  commencing  to  shift  negatively.  F 
and  G.  35  and  40  sec.  after  start  of  stimulation.  The  paroxysm 
disappears  as  SP  shifts  further  negatively. 


theory  advanced  by  Libet  &  Gerard  (30)  and 
supported  by  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (2).  Such  applied 
polarization  ma\'  be  expected  to  shift  the  charges 
along  the  pyramidal  neurons,  perhaps  increasing  or 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED    WITH    FOREBRAIN    EXCITATION     PRf)f:ESSES 


3^3 


decreasing  the  excitability  of  the  substrate.  By  use  of 
surface-positive  applied  polarization,  a  paroxysm 
can  be  initiated  in  the  rabbit  at  a  significantly  lower 
intensity  than  is  required  to  produce  a  paroxysm 
with  surface-negative  polarization.  Such  a  paroxysm 
is  also  accompanied  by  a  positive  .SP  change.  This, 
likewise,  is  supplanted  by  a  negative  one  after  the 
paroxysm  disappears.  If  a  paroxysm  is  initiated  by 
sufficiently  strong  surface-negative  polarization,  that 
paroxysm  is  also  related  to  a  surface-positi\'e  SP  shift 
which  develops  in  reaction  to  the  immediately  pre- 
ceding surface-negative   applied   polarization. 

More  recent  studies  (16)  have  re\ealed  an  SP 
change  accompanying  a  cortical  paroxysm  induced 
by  ventroanterior  thalamic  stimulation  opposite  to 
the  one  initiated  by  the  methods  cited  abo\e  (stim- 
ulation of  relay  nucleus  and  polarization).  In  this 
instance  the  .SP  shifts  negatively  durina;  the  high 
\oltage  discharge  and  then  positively  in  the  post- 
paroxysmal  depression   period  (fig.    10).   A   cortical 


PIG.  10.  D.C.  change  accompanying  a  cortical  paroxysm 
induced  by  repetitive  stimulation  of  ventroanterior  thalamic 
nucleus  in  the  cat.  A.  Negative  d.c.  shift  with  20  per  sec.  stimu- 
lation. .\  3  sec.  strip  of  record  has  been  omitted  between  A  and 
B;  with  continuation  of  stimulation  there  is  an  increase  in 
negative  d.c.  change.  C.  Upon  cessation  of  stimulation  (white 
dot)  high  voltage  paro.xysmal  activity  is  in  evidence  and  the 
steady  potential  remains  shifted  negatively.  D.  Return  of 
steady  potential  to  the  prestimulatory  base  line  with  termination 
of  paro.xysm.  E.  Positive  shift  of  steady  potential  in  the  post- 
stimulatory  isoelectric  period.  Vertical  line  of  right  angle  in 
right  lower  corner  represents  500  mv;  horizontal  line,  i  sec. 
Positive  is  up. 


FIG.  II.  .SP  shift  accompanying  cortical  paro.xysm  induced 
by  thiocarbohydrazide  in  rabbit.  .Straight  white  lines  are  base 
lines  from  which  shifts  in  SP  are  read.  Positive  is  up.  A.  15  min. 
after  intravenous  injection  of  30  mg  of  thiocarbohydrazide. 
SP  commences  to  shift  negatively.  B.  10  sec.  later  SP  shifts 
more  negatively  and  paroxysmal  activity  begins.  C.  6  sec. 
later  paro.xysmal  activity  continues  and  SP  remains  shifted 
negatively.  D  and  E.  As  high  voltage  discharge  breaks  up  and 
stops,  SP  shifts  back  to  the  base  line.  The  tracing  nearest  the 
base  line  in  D  is  the  same  paroxysm  recorded  at  lower  ampli- 
fication on  the  b  beam  of  the  oscilloscope.  This  beam  is  set  at 
lower  gain  in  order  to  record  the  full  excursions  of  larger  SP 
shifts.  In  the  10  sec.  interval  between  C  and  D,  the  a  beam 
threatened  to  move  off  the  tube  face  and  therefore  the  b  beam 
was  turned  on.  In  all  other  strips  only  the  a  beam  is  shown 
There  is  a  10  sec.  interval  between  D  and  E. 


paroxvsm  initiated  by  the  intravenous  injection  of 
convulsive  drugs  such  as  thiocarbohydrazide  and 
pentylenetetrazol  (Metrazol)  is  accompanied  by  a 
similar  d.c.  change  as  shown  in  figure  1 1  (Goldring, 
S.,  P.  \'anasupa  &  J.  L.  O'Leary,  manuscript  in 
preparation).  Other  workers  have  also  demonstrated 
SP  shifts  accompanying  paroxysmal  activity,  van 
Harreveld  &  Stamm  (43)  found  a  negative  SP  shift 
with  cortical  paroxysm  produced  by  faradic  stimula- 
tion of  the  cortical  surface  or  intravenous  injection  of 
pentylenetetrazol,  and  Liberson,  using  the  guinea 
pig,  found  SP  shifts  accompanying  induced  par- 
oxysmal   discharge    in    the    hippocampus   (29,    44). 

D.C.  Changes  Which  Accompany  Spreading  Depression  (5D) 

In  1944  Leao  (24)  discovered  a  depression  of  the 
usual  cortical  rhvthms  of  the  rabbit  which  spreads 


324 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


slowly  outwards  from  the  site  of  a  weak  mechanical, 
electrical  or  chemical  stimulus  to  the  cortex.  This 
reaction  was  elicited  more  readily  from  the  rostral 
pole  of  the  hemisphere,  and  from  there  a  wave  of 
depression  could  envelop  almost  the  whole  of  the  con- 
vexity, propagating  at  a  velocity  of  only  2  to  5  mm 
per  min.  Neither  .evoked  potentials  nor  motor  re- 
sponses to  cortical  stimulation  could  be  observed  when 
the  front  of  the  depression  reached  the  sensorimotor 
cortex.  Leao  also  observed  that  the  propagation  of  the 
SD  was  accompanied  by  dilation  of  pial  vessels  (25), 
an  observation  confirmed  by  van  Harreveld  &  Ochs 
(42)  who  also  held  that  in  the  rabbit  the  vasodila- 
tation is  preceded  by  a  smaller  wave  of  vasocon- 
striction. Species  differences  in  cortical  su.sceptibility 
to  SD  have  been  noted.  It  is  produced  more  easily 
in  the  rabbit  than  in  the  cat  and  is  seen  only  occasion- 
ally in  the  monkey  (33,  36,  45).  Cortical  maturity 
may  also  play  a  role:  Bures  (3)  was  unable  to  obtain 
SD  during  the  first  days  of  life  in  the  rat,  although 
he  could  elicit  it  readily  in  the  newborn  guinea  pig. 
There  is  substantial  evidence  indicating  that  SD 
is  an  abnormal  reaction  which  results  from  exposure 
of  the  brain  to  unphysiological  conditions.  Marshall 
and  co-workers  (32,  34,  35,  36)  have  shown  that  SD 
appears  consistently  only  if  the  brain  has  been  de- 
hydrated, exposed  to  the  atmosphere  for  long  periods, 
cooled,   or   bathed    in    Ringer's  .solution   having   ten 


times  the  usual  concentration  of  potassium.  In  the 
absence  of  one  or  another  of  the  above  conditions 
they  were  unable  to  elicit  SD  in  the  cat  or  monkey  at 
all,  although  occasionally  it  could  still  be  developed 
in  the  rabbit.  Because  they  were  able  to  record  SD 
through  the  intact  dura  of  the  rabbit,  van  Harreveld 
rt  al.  (45)  disagreed  with  the  view  that  SD  is  an  ab- 
normal reaction.  However,  later,  utilizing  the  various 
conditions  described  by  Marshall  and  co-workers, 
van  Harreveld  &  Bogen  (40)  obtained  SD  in  the  area 
retrosplenialis  granularis  dorsalis,  a  region  into  which 
SD  does  not  propagate  under  usual  recording  con- 
ditions in  the  rabljit. 

The  d.c.  variation  which  accompanies  SD  has  a 
duration  of  4  to  6  min.  (26).  With  the  critical  record- 
ing electrode  placed  upon  the  pial  surface  an  involved 
cortical  region  becomes  negative  for  i  to  2  min.  with 
respect  to  a  subcortical  or  an  extracortical  reference 
electrode.  Within  that  time  the  surface-negativity 
reaches  a  maximum  of  8  to  15  mv,  thereafter  decreas- 
ing with  somewhat  greater  rapidity.  The  involved 
region  then  becomes  3  to  8  mv  surface-positive  but 
returns  to  the  predepression  base  line  in  3  to  5  min. 
(fig.  12).  As  the  d.c.  potential  changes  from  surface- 
negative  to  positive,  large  amplitude  (2  to  3  mv) 
negative  slow  waves  or  the  repetitive  spikes  of  con- 
vulsive discharge  may  occur.  The  occurrence  of  these 
transients  or  repetitive  spikes  led  Leao  to  the  con- 


8   MIN.  9 


FIG.  12.  A  representative  experiment  on  the  slow  voltage  variation  accompanying  the  spreading 
depression  of  activity.  The  curve  was  drawn  from  voltage  readings  taken  with  hve  sec.  intervals.  In 
this  and  the  other  two  figures,  an  upward  deflection  denotes  negativity  of  the  corte.x  with  respect  to 
the  extracortical  reference  electrode.  Electrodes  arranged  as  shown  in  the  inset  (s:  stimulating 
electrodes).  Stimulation,  5  sec.  of  'tetanizing'  current  from  an  induction  coil,  delivered  at  the  time 
marked  S.  In  this  representative  curve  there  is  indicated  the  time  of  occurrence  (from  .\  to  B)  of  the 
specific  electrical  activity  which  often  develops  during  the  depression  of  the  spontaneous  pat- 
terns.' [From  Leao  (26).] 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED    WITH    FOREBRAIN    EXCITATION    PROCESSES 


325 


elusion  that  SD  and  the  spread  of  convulsive  discharge 
may  be  closely  related. 

By  recording  the  d.c.  change  at  successive  cortical 
depths  Leao  (27)  was  able  to  show  that  the  negative 
shift  appeared  later  at  an  intracortical  electrode  than 
at  one  directly  above  it  on  the  surface.  Also,  when  an 
electrode  upon  the  pial  surface  or  in  the  superficial 
cortex  recorded  a  significant  negative  variation,  a 
deeper  cortical  electrode  showed  a  positive  variation. 
From  these  observations  Leao  concluded  that  SD 
starts  in  the  superficial  cortex  and  is  propagated  down- 
ward to  involve  the  entire  cortical  thickness.  Freygang 
&  Landau  (9)  reached  the  same  conclusion. 

A  negative  voltage  variation  similar  to  the  one 
which  accompanies  SD  has  been  observed  with 
cortical  anemia,  anexia  and  asphyxia  (11,  17,  26,  43), 
a  fact  suggesting  neuronal  depolarization  as  an  im- 
portant contributory  factor  in  all.  Extrapolating  from 
results  obtained  during  depolarization  in  peripheral 
nerve  (6,  7),  one  would  expect  decreased  cortical 
impedance  to  result  from  both  SD  and  cortical  anoxia. 
However,  several  workers  (9,  28,  41)  have  reported 
the  opposite  occurrence.  Freygang  &  Landau  (9) 
have  suggested  that  swelling  of  the  neuronal  and  glial 
elements  of  cortex  may  account  for  the  increased 
cortical  impedance  which  accompanies  SD  and  cor- 
tical anoxia,  arguing  that  cellular  swelling  would 
increase  the  resistance  of  the  extracellular  current 
shunt  and  thus  the  tissue  resistance,  van  Harreveld 
&  Ochs  (41)  have  agreed  with  this  view.  However, 
recent  studies  of  nervous  tissue  with  the  electron 
microscope  (31)  have  failed  to  reveal  the  existence 
of  an  extracellular  space  in  the  cortex,  thus  .suggesting 
that  other  explanations  need  also  be  entertained.  On 
the  other  hand  van  Harreveld  (39)  has  reported 
direct  confirmation  of  swelling  in  the  superficial 
dendritic  plexus  during  SD,  using  histological  sections 
prepared    after    freezing. 

Grafstein's  (21)  recent  observations  with  micro- 
electrodes  are  also  important.  She  noted  increa.sed 
firing  of  single  units  at  the  start  of  SD,  sugs;esting  some 
other  explanation  for  the  depression  of  EGG  con- 
ventionally recorded  with  macroelectrodes.  Asyn- 
chronous firing  of  single  units  leading  to  cancellation 
of  opposing  effects  could,  however,  reconcile  macro- 
and  microelectrode  results.  Grafstein's  observations 
have  also  led  her  to  implicate  depolarization  resulting 
from  the  liberation  of  pota.ssium  as  the  cau.se  of  SD. 
Thus,  the  increased  neuronal  discharge  shown  by 
microelectrode  studies  could  result  in  decreased  cell 
permeability  (depolarization)  and   the  liberation  of 


potassium.  The  latter  could  chemicalh-  stimulate 
adjoining  cells,  the  process  spreading  to  involve  the 
entire   cortex. 

Relalion  oj  Polarity  oj  Evoked  Traiisiiml  to  Polarity 
of  SP  Change  Also  Consequent  to  Stimulation 

Much  indirect  evidence  has  been  presented  here 
indicating;  that  the  same  neuronal  activity  occasions 
both  the  transient  of  the  conventional  EGG  and  the 
SP  changes  described.  Direct  proof  is  also  needed  and 
is  possible  to  obtain  in  an  experimental  situation 
which  provides  a  layer  of  neurons  synapticallv  ac- 
tivated from  one  surface  with  impulses  conducted 
away  from  the  other.  The  lateral  geniculate  nucleus 
of  the  cat  was  studied  by  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (2)  who 
showed  that  the  postsynaptic  spike  recorded  from  a 
critical  electrode  in  the  optic  radiation  over  the  genic- 
ulate cell  layers  has  a  positive  polarity,  and  that,  as 
the  critical  electrode  enters  the  cell  layers,  the  polarity 
reverses  to  negative.  They  concluded  that,  with  regard 
to  evoked  potentials  of  the  lateral  geniculate,  the  cell 
body  during  activity  becomes  negative  to  its  own  con- 
ducting axon.  Vastola  (46)  undertook  to  repeat  this 
experiment,  determining  the  reversal  point  of  the 
evoked  transient  from  the  same  electrode  used  to 
record  the  summated  SP  shift  which  accompanies 
rapid  repetitive  stimulation.  For  this  purpose  he  used 
glass  capillary  tube  electrodes  having  a  tip  diameter  of 
250  ;u  and  led  from  calomel  cells.  The  critical  electrode 
was  passed  from  the  optic  radiation  through  the  cell 
layers,  the  reference  electrode  being  situated  in  the 
central  white  matter  anterior  to  the  lateral  geniculate 
body.  He  deterinined  the  maximal  evoked  response 
transient  obtainable  from  a  single  shock  applied  to 
the  contralateral  optic  nerve,  and  then  proceeded  to 
study,  at  different  strengths  of  stimulation  between 
threshold  and  maximum,  the  SP  shift  which  occurs 
concurrently  with  stimulation  to  150  per  sec.  Dorsal 
to  the  cell  layers  SP  became  positively  shifted  during 
repetitive  stimulation;  at  0.5  mm  dorsal  to  the  cell 
layers  the  SP  shift  accompanying  repetitive  stimula- 
tion reversed  polarity;  with  increasing  depth  of  the 
critical  electrode  the  negative  shift  increased  further 
until  the  electrode  tip  was  in  the  middle  of  the  first 
layer  of  the  nucleus.  Then  it  gradually  decreased  as 
the  electrode  passed  through  the  remaining  cell 
layers  and  into  the  thalamus  ventral  to  the  nucleus. 
The  polarity  of  the  SP  shift  coincided  with  the  polarity 
of  the  postsynaptic  wave  which  he  recorded  by  the 
conventional  single  shock  method.  As  an  added  pre- 


326 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH^SIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


caution  \'astola  used  the  chopper  technique  at  a  fast 
rate  of  interruption  to  prove  that  the  SP  between  the 
indisidual  transients  also  shifted  negatively. 

Effects  uf  Stimulation  at  a  Distance  Along  a 
Mullisynaplic  Path  Upon  Transcortical  SP 

Dondey  &  Snider  (8)  recorded  SP  from  the  cerebral 
cortex  in  much  the  same  fashion  as  outlined  herein, 
using  animals  prepared  under  ether  anesthesia  and 
maintained  under  a'-tubocurarine  (15  mg  per  kg). 
Besides  confirming  the  findings  reported  previously 
concerning  the  relation  between  the  appearance  of 
cortical  paroxysm  and  of  positive  SP  shift,  and  be- 
tween the  postictal  silent  period  and  negative  shift, 
they  studied  the  effect  of  fast  frequency  cerebellar 
stimulation (200  and  300  per  sec,  10  to  30  v.).  Such 
stimulation  induced  a  positive  shift  in  cortical  SP  last- 
ing for  as  long  as  50  sec.  and  becoming  as  large  as  5 
mv.  Suppression  of  cortical  spindles  occurred  during 
the  shift  and  was  the  principal  criterion  for  the  effi- 
cacy of  the  cerebellar  stimulation.  Under  the  same 
circumstances  slow  frequency  stimulation,  ranging 
between  10  and  20  per  sec,  induced  a  negative  SP 
shift  in  the  cortex  which  might  last  as  long  as  70  sec. 
and  reach  4  mv  in  amplitude.  With  the  lower  frequency 
stimulation  the  ECG  did  not  change  as  significantly 
and  spindles  might  occur  throughout  the  recording. 
In  both  the  instances  of  fast  and  of  slow  stimulation, 
SP  recording  from  the  nucleus  ventralis  lateralis  of 
the  thalamus  showed  oppositely  polarized  effects.  Don- 
dey &  Snider  found  in  addition  that  fast  frequency 
stimulation  of  the  cerebellum  might  prevent  the  ex- 
pected negative  shift  which  occurs  at  the  end  of  cor- 
tically  induced  paroxysmal  discharge;  instead  posi- 
tivity  continued  much  beyond  the  cessation  of  the 
paroxysm. 

Injury  Potential  Components 

D.C.  changes  with  injury  were  reported  h\  Walker 
et  al.  (47)  and  Meyer  &  Denny-Brown  (37,  38).  K.em- 
pinsky's  (23)  study  of  the  distribution  of  SP 
change  associated  with  experimental  vascular  occlu- 
sion of  the  middle  cerebral  artery  in  the  cat  con- 
clusively demonstrates  that  a  significant  component 
of  such  injury  effect  is  a  demarcation  potential  across 
the  zone  of  injury  in  the  white  matter.  He  used  pia- 
ventricular,  subcortical-ventricular  and  transcortical 
leads  simultaneously.  The  prompt,  sustained  negative 
shift  which  he  obtained  in  the  center  of  the  cortical 


area  of  distribution  of  the  vessel  could  be  recorded 
in  the  subcortical-ventricular  and  the  pia-ventricular 
combinations  but  not  in  the  cortex-subcortex  one. 
When  recorded  simultaneously  at  different  cortical 
loci,  the  magnitude  of  the  shift  decremented  toward 
the  periphery  of  the  ischemic  region. 

Human  Studies  I'smg  Scalp  Recording 

In  a  study  by  Goldring  et  al.  (19)  it  was  shown  that 
d.c  changes  can  also  be  recorded  from  the  human 
scalp  during  electroconvulsive  therapy.  The  difiiculty 
which  arises  in  the  interpretation  of  these  and  other 
d.c.  changes  recorded  from  the  scalp  with  non- 
polarizable  electrodes  is  the  complication  introduced 
by  d.c.  changes  which  occur  in  the  skin.  Of  the  results 
reported  in  the  literature  the  negative  shift  with  3 
per  sec.  spike  and  dome  discharges  (5)  seems  most 
free  of  this  criticism. 


DISCUSSION   OF  ORIGIN   OF  STE.ADY   POTENTI.iiLS 

Starting  from  a  base  of  transcortical  voltage  meas- 
urement called  steady  potential  (SP),  evidence  thus 
far  accumulated  supports  the  existence  of  d.c.  con- 
comitants of  conventionally  recorded  cortical  excita- 
tion processes.  The  d.c.  changes  which  correspond 
have  also  been  obtained  from  some  subcortical  centers. 
In  one  such  nucleus  (the  lateral  geniculate)  it  seems 
clear  that  there  is  a  close  association  between  d.c. 
change  and  the  neural  excitation  process  (46).  In 
the  cortex  d.c.  changes  occur  subsequent  to  brief 
transients  such  as  evoked  responses,  strychnine  and 
veratrine  spikes,  and  during  and  after  repetitive  ones 
like  barbiturate  spindles  or  recruiting  responses. 
Thus,  bv  means  of  a  d.c.  amplifier  the  electrical  sign 
of  excitation  at  a  cortical  point  can  be  recorded  trans- 
corticalK'  significantK'  after  the  acti\ity  obtained  with 
a  condensor-coupled  amplifier  has  disappeared. 

The  evidence  is  yet  insufficient  to  decide  whether 
such  d.c.  changes  are  simply  a  prolongation  of  the 
same  neural  process  which  occasions  a  neuron's  dis- 
charge or  are  analogous  to  the  after-potentials  of 
peripheral  nerve.  If  the  latter,  evidence  for  metabolic 
causation  needs  to  ije  considered.  There  are  also  in- 
dications, liut  no  proof,  for  excitability  change  occur- 
ring simultaneou.slv  with  certain  after-effects,  and 
this  also  needs  close  investigation.  If  excitability 
change  does  occur,  the  analogy  with  peripheral 
nerve  after-potentials  becomes  much  closer. 


CHANGES    ASSOCIATED    WITH    FOREBRAI.N'    EXCITATION    PROCESSES 


327 


After-effects  appear  to  sum  algeliraically,  giving 
different  polarity  under  different  conditions  of  excita- 
tion. As  used  here  the  term  algebraic  summation 
implies  something  more  than  the  simple  cancellation 
of  opposing  equal  forces.  Such  an  explanation  might 
suffice  for  the  absence  of  detectable  after-effect  early 
in  the  course  of  some  experiments,  but  a  somewhat 
more  complicated  interpretation  is  needed  to  explain 
all  of  the  observed  phenomena  of  after-effect  summa- 
tion during  repetiti\e  processes.  An  increasingly 
negative  after-effect  during  summation  might  thus 
be  due  either  to  exaggeration  of  a  negatively  directed 
tendency,  or  to  suppression  of  a  positively  directed 
one  and  vice  versa.  The  plasticity  of  interaction  be- 
tween the  two  forces  is  exemplified  in  the  summation 
of  cortical  evoked  response  after-effects  induced  by 
repetitive  stimulation  of  the  opposite  optic  nerve,  as 
compared  to  that  of  the  lateral  geniculate  nucleus. 
Optic  nerve  stimulation  in  our  experience  can  cause 
only  a  summation  of  positive  after-effect  when  applied 
at  maximum,  if  at  all;  most  usually  the  summation  is 
that  of  negative  after-effect.  Geniculate  stimulation 
produces  summated  after-effect  consistently  at  stimu- 
lus values  below  maximum.  Another  indication  that 
the  same  neuronal  firing  can  produce  negative  after- 
effect in  one  situation  and  positive  in  another  lies  in 
the  comparison  between  the  summed  negative  after- 
effect of  clusters  of  strychnine  spikes,  and  the  positive 
summation  which  accompanies  the  high  voltage 
paroxysm  that  develops  intermittently  in  the  same 
strychninized  corte.x. 

The  unifying  concept  which  promises  the  most  aid 
in  harmonizing  knowledge  of  the  electrical  signs  of 
the  quicker  transients  with  those  of  the  slower  d.c. 
concomitants  of  neural  excitation  is  that  of  Libet  & 
Gerard  (30).  These  writers  postulated  a  polarization 
gradient  along  the  vertically  oriented  cortical  neurons. 
For  each  neuron  this  gradient  would  extend  from  the 
surface  dendritic  expansions  in  the  plexiform  layer  to 
a  deeper  level,  even  to  layer  \T,  where  the  soma-axon 
junction  is  situated.  Further  evidence  relating 
polarization  gradients  to  neural  excitation  is  obtained 
by  the  simple  expedient  of  changing  the  charge  dis- 
tribution along  the  pyramidal  cells  artificially.  This 
affects  significantly  the  positive  and  negative  com- 
ponents of  evoked  potential,  surface-negative  polari- 
zation accentuating  the  positive  phase,  surface-positive 
polarization  the    negative    phase. 

Other  support  for  Libet  &  Gerard's  view  is  to  be 
found  in  the  change  in  visual  esoked  response  during 
the  cycles  of  intense  negative  d.c.  shift  which  char- 


acterizes the  veratrinized  cortex.  The  effect  of  vera- 
trine  applied  to  the  cortical  surface  is  to  depolarize 
the  terminal  dendritic  brushes  of  the  cortical  neurons 
(13,  14),  and  from  this  depolarized  superficial  region 
intense  waves  of  depolarization  appear  to  spread 
downwards  over  the  cortical  dendrites,  progressively 
engulfing  the  cortical  neurons  from  above.  The 
initial  positive  phase  of  evoked  response  is  due  to  the 
successive  activation  of  groups  of  neurons,  each  ex- 
cited after  synaptic  conduction.  The  summation  pro- 
duced is  signified  by  the  four  fast  spikes  which  appear 
successively  higher  upon  the  rising  phase  of  the  evoked 
potential.  During  the  intense  negative  veratrine  shift 
these  spikes  are  removed  successively  from  above 
downwards,  and  the  order  of  their  removal  corre- 
sponds with  the  expectancy  from  their  positions  in  the 
cortical  depth  as  obtained  by  the  null  point  of  meas- 
urements of  Bishop  &  Clare  (i). 

The  changes  in  charge  distribution  may  actually 
be  more  complicated  than  is  suggested  by  the  results 
of  these  experimental  studies.  Besides  the  situation  in 
which  the  charge  at  the  two  ends  might  change  in 
opposite  direction  to  produce  an  SP  effect,  a  d.c. 
change  might  also  ensue  if  both  ends  changed  in  the 
same  direction  unequally.  Such  a  concept  is  now  sus- 
ceptible to  experimental  proof  only  from  the  limited 
aspect  of  charge  distribution  along  the  neuron's  ex- 
terior. If  interior  differences  of  potential  exist  be- 
tween the  superficial  dendrites  and  the  cell  soma,  it 
will  require  microelectrode  recording  to  reveal  them. 


SUMM.\RY 

Present  methods  of  recording  the  d.c.  potential 
across  the  cerebral  cortex  are  presented.  These  re- 
quire detailed  attention  to  oxygen  tension,  anesthesia 
and  injury,  and  necessitate  stable  electrodes  for  re- 
cording purposes.  The  SP  across  the  cortex  remains 
relatively  steady  under  good  experimental  conditions 
and  serves  as  a  base  line  for  examining  slower  con- 
comitants of  neural  excitation.  In  several  situations  it 
has  been  shown  that  such  SP  concomitants  do  relate 
to  the  quick  transient  phenomena  conventionally 
recorded  from  the  cortex.  Significant  summation  of 
d.c.  change  associated  with  single  transients  can 
appear  during  repetitive  phenomena.  The  best  prom- 
ise of  relating  d.c.  and  transient  phenomena  is  in 
terms  of  the  polarization  theory  of  Libet  &  Gerard 


C30). 


328  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 

REFERENCES 


1.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  M.  H.   Clare.  J.  Neurophysiol.    16:   i, 

1953- 

2.  Bishop,   G.    H.    and  J.    L.    O'Learv.    Elrclroencephalog.  & 

Clin.  Neurophystol.   1:   401,    1950. 

3.  BuRES,  J.  Electroencephalog.&  Clin.  Neurophysiol,  9:  121,  1957. 

4.  BuRGE,  W.  E.,  G.  C.  WicKwiRE,  O.  S.  Orth,  H.  W.  Neild 
AND  W.  p.  Elhardt.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  116:  ig,  1936. 

5.  CoHN,  R.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  71  :  699,  1954. 

6.  Cole,  K.  S.  and  R.  F.  Baker.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24;  535,  1941 . 

7.  Cole,  K.  S.  .^nd  H.  ].  Curtis.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  551, 

■94'- 

8.  Dondey,  M.  and  R.  S.  Snider.   Eleclroencephahg.  &  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  7;  243,  1955. 

9.  Freyoang,  W.  H.,  Jr.  and  \V.   M.   Landau.  J.  Cell.  & 
Comp.  Physiol.  45:  377,  1955. 

10.  Goldensohn,  E.  S.,  M.  D.  Shoenfeld  and  P.  F.  Hoefer. 
Electromcephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  3:  231,   1951. 

11.  Goldring,  S.  and  J.  L.  O'Leary.  J.  Neurophystol.  14:  275, 

'951- 

12.  Goldring,  S.  and  J.  L.  O'Leary.  Eleclroencephahg.  &  Clin. 

Neurophysiol.  3:  329,  1951. 

13.  Goldring,   S.    and  J.    L.    O'Leary. 
Clin.   .Neurophysiol.   6;    189,    1954. 

14.  Goldring,   S.    .and    J.    L.    O'Leary. 
Clin.   Neurophysiol.    6:   201,    1954. 

15.  Goldring,   S.    and  J.    L.    O'Leary. 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  9:  381,  1957. 

16.  Goldring,   S.    and  J.    L.    O'Leary. 
Clin.    .Neurophysiol.  9:  577,  1957. 

17.  Goldring,  S.,  J.  L.  O'Leary  and  R.  B.   King.  Eleclro- 
encephahg. &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  10:  233,  1958. 

18.  Goldring,   S.,  J.    L.   O'Leary   and   R.    L.    Lam.    Eleclro- 
encephahg. &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  5-  395,   1953. 

19.  Goldring,    S.,     G.     Ulett,    J.     L.     O'Leary    and    A. 
Greditzer.   Electroenccphalog.  &   Clin.   Neurophysiol.   2 :   297, 

1950- 

21.  Grafstein,  B.  J.  Neurophysiol.  19:  154,  1956. 

22.  Jasper,  H.   and  T.  C.   Erickson.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  7:  333, 

1944- 

23.  Kempinsky,   W.   H.   Eleclroencephahg.  G?   Clin.  Neurophysiol. 

6:  375.    1954- 


Eleclroencephahg.  tGf 

Eleclroencephahg.  <^ 

Eleclroencephahg.  & 

Eleclroencephahg.  C^ 


24.  Leao,  a.  a.  p.  J.  Neurophysiol.  7:  359,   1944. 

25.  Leao,  A.  A.  P.  J.  Neurophysiol.  7;  391,  1944. 

26.  Leao,  A.  A.  P.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   10:  409,   1947. 

27.  Leao,   A.   A.    P.   Eleclroencephahg.  &   Clin.   .Neurophysiol.    3: 

315.  I95I- 

28.  Leao,  A.  A.  P.  and  H.  M.  Ferriera.  .In.  .Acad.  Brasileira 
Cien.    25:   259,    1953. 

29.  LiBERSON,  W.  T.  Am.  J.  Psychiat.   112:  91,   1955. 

30.  Libet,  B.  and  R.  VV.  Gerard.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  438,  1941. 

31.  Luse,  S.  a.  J.  Biopkys.  &  Biochem.  Cytol.  2:  531,  1956. 

32.  Marshall,   W.    H.   Eleclroencephahg.  &   Clin.   Neurophysiol. 

2:   177.   1950- 

33.  Marshall,   VV.   H.   Eleclroencephahg.  Si   Clin.   Neurophysiol. 
4:  223,   1952. 

34.  Marshall,   VV.    H.    Eleclroencephahg.  &   Clin.  .Neurophysiol. 
Suppl.  4:  82,   1953. 

35.  MARSH.'kLL,   VV.   H.   and   C.   F.   Essig.  J.  Neurophysiol.    14' 
265,    1 95 1. 

36.  Marshall,  VV.  H.,  C.   F.  Essig  and  S.  J.  Dubroff.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  14:  153,  1951. 

37.  Meyer,  J.  S.   and  D.  Denny-Brown.  Eleclroencephahg.  <S 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  7:511,  1955. 

38.  Me^er,  J.  S.  and  p.  Denny-Brown.  Eleclroencephahg.  & 
Clin.    Neurophysiol.    7:    529,    1955. 

39.  van  Harreveld,  a.  Fed.  Proc.    16:   131,   1957. 

40.  VAN  Harreveld,   A.   and  J.   E.   Bogen.   Proc.  Soc.  Exper. 
Biol.  &  Med.  91  :  297,   1956. 

41.  van   Harreveld,   .A.   and  S.   Ochs.   .4m.  J.   Physiol.    187: 
180,  1956. 

42.  van  Harreveld,  A.  and  S.  Ochs.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  189:  159, 

■957- 

43.  van  Harreveld,  A.  .\nd  J.  S.  Stamm.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.  173: 

■71.  '953- 

44.  VAN  Harreveld,  A.  and  J.  S.  Stamm.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   17: 

505.  '954- 

45.  VAN  Harreveld,  A.,  J.   S.   St.\mm  and   E.   Christensen. 
Am.  J.  Physiol.    184:  312,   1956. 

46.  Vastola,   E.   F.   Eleclroencephahg.  &   Clin.  .Neurophysiol.    7 : 

557.  1955- 

47.  W.-^LKER,  A.  E.,  J.  J.  Kellross  and  T.  J.  C.^se.  J.  Nemo- 
surg.    I  :   1 03,    1 944. 


CHAPTER   XIV 


The  physiopathology  of  epileptic  seizures 

HENRI    GAS  TAUT     I     Favulte  de  Medecine,  Marseille,  France 
M.    F  I  S  C  H  E  R -  W  I  L  L  I  A  M  S     j     London  Hospital,  London,  England 


CHAPTER    CONTENTS 

Symptomatology 

Types  of  Generalized  Epilepsy 
Grand  mal 

Petit  mal  of  absence'  type 
Petit  mal  of  myoclonic  type 
Types  of  Partial  Epilepsy 
Clinical  aspects 

Electroencephalographic  aspects 
Etiology 

Functional  Epilepsy 
Organic  Epilepsy 
Physiopathology  of  Seizures  Generalized  from  Start 
Experimental  Results 
Grand  mal 

Theories  of  generalized  convulsions 

Mechanism  of  bioelectric  discharges  in  generalized  epilepsy 
Petit  mal  of  myoclonic  type 
Petit  mal  of  'absence'  type 
Interpretation  of  Experimental  Results 

Origin,  nature  and  propagation  of  nervous  acti\ity  respon- 
sible for  generalized  grand  mal  seizure 
Causes  of  reticular  discharges,  thalamic  and  niesenceph- 
alorhombencephalic,  responsible  for  grand  mal  epilepsy 
Duration    and    termination    of   discharge    in    generalized 

epilepsy 
Myoclonus  of  petit  mal 
Petit  mal  'absence' 
Physiopathology  of  Partial  Epilepsies 
Experimental  Results 

Experimental    partial    epilepsy    of    cortical    origin 
Experimental  partial  epilepsy  of  rhinencephalic  origin 
Experimental  partial  epilepsy  of  subcortical  origin 
Experimental    partial    epilepsy,    secondarily    generalized 
Experimental  partial  epilepsy  with  erratic  discharges 
Anatomicals  Studies 
Physiopathogenesis  of  Partial  Epilepsies 

Origin  and  cause  of  neuronal  discharge  in  partial  epilepsy 
Propagation    and    termination   of  neuronal   discharge   in 

partial  epilepsy 
Distinction  between  two  great  varieties  of  partial  epilepsy 
with  respect  to  character  of  their  discharges 


BECAUSE  OF  THE  IMPORTANT  mcdical  application 
of  this  chapter,  it  seems  useful  to  preface  it  with  a 
short  summary  of  the  clinical  and  electroenceph- 
alographic events  accompanying  epileptic  seizures 
in    man. 

SYMPTOMATOLOGY 

There  are  two  types  of  epilepsy,  from  both  the 
clinical  and  the  electroencephalographic  point  of 
view:  a)  generalized  epilepsy,  in  which  the  clinical 
manifestations  involve  the  entire  individual  and  the 
EEG  discharges  can  be  recorded  all  over  the  scalp; 
and  6)  partial  epilepsy,  in  which  only  a  part  of  the 
individual  is  involved  clinically  and  the  electrical 
disturbance  can  be  recorded  from  a  part  of  the  scalp 
only. 

Types  oj  Generalized  Epilepsy 

It  is  clear  that  the  different  types  of  generalized 
epilepsy  arc  similar  in  their  electrical  and  clinical 
manifestations;  they  constitute  a  remarkably  homoge- 
neous group  in  symptomatology.  From  the  clinical 
point  of  view,  the  two  important  features  which  they 
have  in  common  are  loss  of  consciousness  and  con- 
vulsions involving  the  whole  skeletal  musculature  to 
a  greater  or  lesser  extent.  These  phenomena  are  so 
spectacular  that  the  other  manifestations,  notably 
those  in  the  autonomic  sphere,  are  relegated  to  .second 
place.  From  the  EEG  point  of  view,  their  common 
manifestation  is  a  seizure  discharge  of  conxulsive 
waves  which  are  bilateral,  synchronous,  symmetrical 
and  generalized  over  the  scalp.  The  following  three 
varieties    of   generalized    epilepsy    are    distinguished 


329 


33° 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEt'ROPH'YSIOLOCY 


accordiiiE;  to  the  duration  of  the  attack,  to  the  relative 
importance  of  motor  or  mental  symptoms  and  to  the 
characteristics  of  the  EEG  discharge. 

GRAND  MAL.  This  is  characterized  by:  a)  duration  of 
more  than  i  min.;  h")  initial  total  loss  of  consciousness 
with  postictal  coma;  c)  generalized  tonic  contraction 
at  first  continuous  and  later  interrupted  by  periods 
of  relaxation  which  causes  the  so-called  'clonic' 
phase;  d)  a  discharge  of  rhythmical,  bilateral,  syn- 
chronous and  symmetrical  spikes  at  lo  ±  2  cps,  the 
amplitude  of  which  increases  while  the  frecjuency 
diminishes  and  in  which  the  terminal  elements,  sepa- 
rated by  intervals  of  electrical  silence,  constitute 
groups,  each  corresponding  to  a  jerk  in  the  clonic 
phase. 

PETIT  M.'^L  OF  '.'^bsencje'  VARIETY'.  This  is  characterized 
by:  a)  a  shorter  duration  (5  to  20  sec);  A)  more  or 
less  complete  loss  of  consciousness  which  is  ne\cr 
followed  by  postictal  coma;  f)  aborti\e  muscular 
contractions,  which  are  hardly  discernible  and  occur 
three  times  a  second,  involving  the  eyelids  and  some- 
times the  muscles  of  the  head  and  upper  limbs;  and 
rf)  a  rhythmical,  bilateral,  synchronous  and  symmetri- 
cal discharge  of  a  complex  pattern,  comprising  a 
spike  followed  by  a  slow  waxe  and  repeated  three 
times  a  second. 

PETIT  MAL  OF  .MYOCLONIC  TYPE.  This  is  characterized 
by:  a)  an  exceedingly  brief  duration  (a  fraction  of  a 
second);  6)  a  single  violent  jerk  which,  though  gen- 
eralized, predominantly  inxoKes  the  muscles  of  the 
arms  or  head  and  sometimes  appears  on  one  side 
only;  and  f)  a  short  burst  of  spikes,  with  or  without 
one  or  several  slow  waves,  and  constituting,  as  the 
case  may  be,  multiple  spikes,  multiple  spikes  and 
waves,  or  even  a  spike  and  wave. 

The  'spikes'  of  grand  and  petit  mal  are  in  reality 
waves  whose  form  and  period  differ  only  slighth  from 
those  which  characterize  the  waves  of  the  alpha 
rhythm  and  have  nothing  to  do  with  the  spikes,  cor- 
rectly so-termed,  of  interictal  discharges  in  partial 
epilepsy. 

Types  of  Partial  Epilepsy 

The.se  forms  of  epilepsy,  b\'  contrast,  constitute  an 
essentially  heterogeneous  group. 


CLINICAL  ASPECTS.  From  the  clinical  point  of  view,  the 
seizures  are  manifested  by  mental,  .sensory  or  motor 
symptoms  in\olving  the  autonomic  or  cerebrospinal 
systems,  o)  The  sensory  symptoms  may  be  classified 
as  somesthetic,  visual,  auditory,  \ertiginous,  olfactory 
or  gustatory.  /))  Mental  symptoms  include  all  degrees 
of  clouded  consciousness  and  also  positi\e  phenomena 
affecting  perception,  ideation  or  mood — illusions  or 
hallucinations,  'forced  thinking'  or  conversely  a  blot- 
ting out  of  thought,  and  feelings  of  anxiety,  fear  or 
anger,  c)  Visceral  symptoms  are  characterized  by 
abnormal  sensations  or  acti\ities  in\oKing  the  ali- 
mentary system  (abdominal  or  epigastric  sensations, 
chewing  with  salivation,  borborygmi,  defecation, 
etc.),  the  cardio\ascular  or  respiratory  systems  (pre- 
cordial pain,  tachycardia,  vasomotor  phenomena, 
cough,  apnea,  etc.),  and  in  addition  but  less  fre- 
cjuently,  symptoms  in\olving  the  glands,  erectores 
pilorum,  sphincters,  pupils,  etc.  (T)  Somatomotor 
symptoms  include  abnormal  tonic  or  clonic  move- 
ments, the  commonest  being  desiation  and  contra- 
version.  Apart  from  these,  there  are  numerous  ab- 
normal gestures  which  are  responses  to  hallucinations 
(gesture  of  fear  during  a  terrifying  vision)  or  to  sensa- 
tions (for  example,  the  gesture  of  placing  the  hand  on 
the  abdomen  associated  with  a  painful  epigastric 
sensation),  or  which  merely  represent  the  release  of 
automatisms  during  an  ictal  or  postictal  confusional 
episode.  Gibljs  et  al.  (88)  proposed  the  term  'psy- 
chomotor' for  seizures  in  which  there  are  gestures 
such  as  these  but  especially  for  attacks  with  con- 
fusional automatisms. 

.An  enumeration  of  symptoms  cannot  ser\'e  as  the 
basis  for  a  cla.ssification  of  the  partial  epilepsies;  it  is 
even  less  satisfactory  relati\e  to  physiopathological 
interpretation.  It  is  indeed  exceptional  for  an  attack 
of  partial  epilepsy  to  be  manifested  by  a  single  symp- 
tom; quite  the  contrary,  most  of  the  seizures  simulta- 
neously involve  various  sensory,  mental  and  motor 
phenomena.  In  addition,  it  is  impossible  to  locate  a 
precise  region  of  the  brain  to  which  the  origin  of  each 
of  the  above-mentioned  symptoms  might  be  assigned. 
The  conception  of  "representation'  is  inisleading  (91) 
when  motor  and  sensory  'representations'  are  said  to  be 
contralateral,  homolateral  or  bilateral,  or  primary, 
secondary  or  supplementary,  and  to  occupy  different 
cortical  and  subcortical  regions.  It  thus  becomes 
impossible  to  relate  salivation,  for  example,  exclu- 
sively to  the  opercular  region,  or  deviation  of  the 
head  and  eyes  simply  to  the  'prcmotor'  region. 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


331 


ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHic  ASPECTS.  Partial  epileptic 
seizures  differ  from  generalized  seizures  in  that  their 
discharges  can  be  recorded  from  a  part  of  the  scalp 
only,  and  they  show  a  great  diversity  of  expression. 
Without  attempting  to  give  a  full  description,  we  may 
divide  them  into  two  main  topographical  groups: 
a)  localized  partial  discharges,  consisting  of  rhsthmic 
spikes,  occasionally  from  the  frontal  or  central  regions, 
but  much  more  frequently  from  the  temporal  or 
parieto-occipital  regions  and  b)  diffuse  partial  dis- 
charges, showing  as  desynchronization  or  slow  hyper- 
synchronization  and  arising  from  all  or  a  part  of  one 
or  both  hemispheres  (usually  the  temporofrontal 
regions). 

In  addition  there  are  numerous  cases  in  which 
localized  and  diffu.se  discharges  both  appear  during 
the  same  seizure  with  a  variety  of  temporospatial 
relations.  The  discharges  appear  either  independently 
or  concomitantly,  and  either  in  or  out  of  phase; 
they  usually  involve  the  anterior  temporal  and  frontal 
or  the  posterior  temporal  and  parieto-occipital  re- 
gions. 

It  is  of  course  well  recognized  that  any  partial 
seizure  may  become  generalized  and  then  present 
the  electrical  and  clinical  characteristics  of  a  grand 
mal  fit,  whether  or  not  it  is  preceded  by  myoclonic 
jerks.  One  must  therefore  carefully  distinguish  be- 
tween fits  which  are  generalized  from  the  start  and 
those  which  become  generalized  after  a  partial 
onset. 


ETIOLOGY 

It  is  generally  recognized  that  epileptic  seizures 
may  be  divided  into  two  main  categories,  according 
to  whether  or  not  there  is  a  demonstrable  brain  lesion. 
One  category  comprises  the  so-called  secondary  or 
symptomatic  epilepsies  arising  from  a  lesion  which  is 
infective,  degenerative,  traumatic,  neoplastic  or 
vascular;  these  constitute  a  well-recognized  and  un- 
disputed entity.  The  etiology  in  the  other  category  has 
always  been  controversial;  some  authors  regard  the.se 
epileptic  fits  as  the  result  of  brain  lesions  which  are 
undemonstrable  and  consider  that  they  should  be 
called  collectively  cryptogenic  epilepsy;  others  believe 
that  they  represent  disordered  metabolism  or  a  fault 
in  cerebral  function,  unassociated  with  any  organic 
abnormality.  They  should  therefore  be  qualified  as 
"functional',  'metabolic'  or  'primitive'  as  opposed  to 
those  that  are  'organic'  or   'secondary'.   This   termi- 


nology unfortunateh-  has  not  been  adhered  to,  and  it 
has  become  customary  to  call  functional  epilepsies 
either  'idiopathic'  or  'essential'.  This  has  given  rise 
to  a  discussion  the  etymological  origin  of  which  has 
gone  unrecognized  but  which  has  caused  divergence 
of  opinion  which  was  more  apparent  than  real.  How- 
ever this  may  ije,  the  existence  of  two  types  of  epilepsy 
is  now  accepted. 

Functiomd  Epilepsy 

This  disorder  is  encountered  in  only  5  per  cent  of 
all  cases,  according  to  Bicard  et  al.  (17).  It  is  also 
known  as  primitive,  essential,  genuine,  idiopathic, 
metabolic,  genetic,  etc.  It  results  from  a  fault  in  the 
functioning  of  the  brain  manifested  b\-  an  epileptic 
'predisposition',  fairly  often  hereditary.  It  is  not  asso- 
ciated with  any  anatomically  detectable  lesion  of  the 
brain  and  it  is  not  accompanied  by  any  interictal 
neurological  or  psychiatric  manifestations;  and  it  is 
always  manifested  by  seizures  (grand  mal  or  petit 
mal)  which  are  generalized  from  the  start  of  the 
attack. 

Organic  Epilepsy 

This  type  is  very  common  (95  per  cent  of  all 
cases).  It  is  also  known  as  symptomatic  or  secondary. 
It  is  caused  by  an  anatomically  recognizable  cerebral 
lesion  and  for  this  reason  it  may  be  associated  with 
neurological  or  psychiatric  abnonnalities  between 
fits;  it  may  develop  on  a  'soil'  already  predisposed  to 
convulsions  and  thus  a  mildly  irritative  lesion  inay  be 
markedly  epileptogenic;  and  only  rarely  is  it  mani- 
fested as  a  primarily  generalized  seizure,  rather  usu- 
ally appearing  as  partial  epilepsy  which  may  or  may 
not  become  secondarily  generalized. 


PHYSIOPATHOLOGY     OF     SEIZURES 
GENER.-VLIZED    FROM   START 

This  presentation  will  be  divided  into  two  parts. 
The  first  will  be  an  impartial  review  with  some  de- 
.scription  of  the  experimental  results  accumulated  over 
nearly  two  centuries  of  effort  to  explain  the  mecha- 
nism of  generalized  seizures,  particularly  those  of  grand 
mal.  The  second  will  furnish  a  personal  interpretation 
of  these  experimental  results,  based  on  modern  neuro- 
physiological  data. 


332 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Experimental  Results 

The  various  types  of  primarily  generalized  epilepsy 
have  been  reproduced  experimentally  in  animals  and 
man. 

GRAND  MAL.  It  is  easy  to  produce  a  grand  mal  seizure 
in  animals  by  any  measure  acting  as  a  difTuse  assault 
on  the  brain  and  causing  a  sufficiently  widespread 
disorder  of  cerebral  metabolism:  l)y  applying  a  strong 
electric  current  to  the  whole  of  the  brain  (trans- 
cranial electroshock),  by  injecting  analeptic  drugs 
such  as  pentylenetetrazol  (Metrazol),  megimide, 
picrotoxin,  absinthol,  by  oxygen  intoxication,  or  by 
sudden  withdrawal  of  sedati\es  in  chronic  experi- 
mental    barbiturate     poisoning. 

On  the  contrary,  it  is  very  difficult  in  these  same 
animals,  to  cause  by  means  of  minimal  and  localized 
measures  seizures  which  are  generalized  from  the 
onset.  Thus  limited  electrical  stimulation  and  mini- 
mal glial  scars  developed  around  the  site  of  injection 
of  aluminum  hydroxide  never  cause  generalized 
epileptic  fits,  although  they  regularly  produce  seizures 
of  partial  epilepsy  which  may  become  generalized 
(84).  In  order  to  produce  a  grand  mal  seizure  from  the 
onset,  one  needs  to  increase  the  severity  and  particu- 
larly the  extent  of  the  local  disturbance  in  these 
experiments,  to  involve  mid-line  structures  or  to  ad- 
minister an  agent  with  a  generalized  subliminal  action. 

In  man  one  obviously  cannot  produce  focal  experi- 
mental cerebral  lesions,  but  it  is  well  established  that, 
as  in  other  animals,  various  measures  with  a  wide- 
spread cerebral  action  will  cause  grand  mal  seizures: 
electroshock  treatment,  pentylenetetrazol,  oxygen 
poisoning,  rapid  withdrawal  of  barbiturates  (particu- 
larlv  in  addicts  of  short-acting  barbiturates),  etc.  All 
these  forms  of  experimental  or  accidental  epilepsy 
in  man  and  animals  are  indeed  comparable  to  spon- 
taneous seizures  of  grand  mal  generalized  from  the 
start,  for  they  always  include  an  immediate  loss  of 
consciousness,  tonic  and  clonic  convulsions,  and  a 
bilateral,  synchronous  and  symmetrical  seizure  dis- 
charge in  the  EEG. 

We  have  not  regarded  as  grand  mal  seizures  those 
which  are  precipitated  in  man  and  animals  by  \arious 
types  of  cerebral  anoxia.  These  seizures  are  tonic, 
lacking  the  clonic  phase,  and  are  accompanied  by 
depression  of  cerebral  electrical  activity  and  not  by 
a  bisynchronous  discharge.  They  are  none  the  less  of 
fundamental  interest  for  the  understanding  of  grand 
mal  epilepsy  and  we  shall  return  to  the  subject  later. 


THEORIES  OF  GENERALIZED  CONVULSIONS,  o)  Subcortical 
theory.  The  idea  that  generalized  convulsions  have  a 
subcortical  origin  has  been  held  by  physicians  ever 
since  ancient  times  for  logical  reasons  which  are  easily 
understood  (187).  In  the  second  century  A.D.,  Galen 
attributed  grand  mal  epilepsy  to  a  'thick  humor'  in 
the  middle  and  posterior  part  of  the  ventricles.  Willis 
in  1682  had  a  similar  conception  when  he  related  the 
fit  to  "a  strong  spasmodic  copula  distilled  from  the 
blood  into  the  brain,  affecting  the  animal  spirits  which 
lie  in  the  middle  of  the  brain,  and  causing  an  explo- 
sion." This  idea  persists  to  our  day  since  Hogner 
[quoted  by  Marinesco  et  al.  (135)]  recently  defended 
the  view  that  the  epileptic  discharge  depends  on  dis- 
tension of  the  third  ventricle  by  excess  formation  of 
cerebrospinal  fluid,  producing  an  excitation  of  the 
centers  around   the  ventricle. 

Experimental  study  of  this  subcortical  theory  was 
begun  in  the  middle  of  the  eighteenth  century  when 
Haller  showed  that  generalized  convulsions  could  be 
proNoked  h\  irritation  of  the  white  matter  in  the 
depths  of  the  brain.  At  the  beginning  of  the  next 
century  Flourens  (1823)  performed  his  famous  experi- 
ments on  the  medulla  oblongata.  This  allowed  Hall 
(96)  to  formulate  his  theory  of  the  medullary  origin  of 
reflex  epilepsy,  which  was  taken  up  by  Schroeder  van 
der  Kolk  in  1859  ('74)  who  concluded  that  "an 
exalted  sensibility  and  excitability  of  the  medulla 
oblongata  is  the  just  cause  of  epilepsy."  As  early  as 
1B38  Nothnagel  provoked  generalized  convulsions  by 
mechanical  stimulation  of  the  medulla  oijlongata, 
and  much  later  Binswanger  (18)  and  Bechterew  (16) 
repeated  these  experiments  using  an  electric  current 
or  a  needle  prick. 

Stimulation  experiments  were  not,  however,  the 
only  ones  supporting  the  subcortical  mechanism  of 
generalized  seizures.  Toward  the  beginning  of  the 
twentieth  century,  a  large  number  of  ablation  experi- 
ments showed  that  the  cerebral  cortex,  and  the  greater 
part  of  the  telencephalon,  diencephalon  and  mesen- 
cephalon, were  not  necessary  for  the  experimental 
production  of  generalized  epilepsy.  \'arious  measures 
were  used  in  these  experiments:  transcranial  applica- 
tion of  an  electric  current  (89,  130,  166,  171,  191); 
cooling  of  the  brain  (44);  and  injections  of  pentylene- 
tetrazol (Metrazol)  (14,  95,  130,  173),  of  insulin 
(126,  153),  of  picrotoxin  (36)  or  of  a  mixture  of 
chloralose  and  strychnine  (144).  Thus  generalized 
convulsions  are  seen  in  the  diencephalic,  the  mesen- 
cephalic  and  e\en   in   the   rhombencephalic   animal 


THE    PHVSIOPATHOLOGV    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


333 


which  possesses  nothing  but  the  medulla  and  pons, 
but  they  no  longer  occur  in  the  spinal  animal.  One 
has  therefore  to  postulate  the  existence  of  an  anatom- 
ical structure  in  the  brain  stem,  which  extends  to  its 
most  caudal  part,  and  which  is  connected  with  the 
spinal  inotor  neurons  and  ai)le  to  transmit  to  them 
convulsant  impulses.  Such  a  structure,  suspected 
by  Lucciani  under  the  name  of  'common  motor 
center'  may,  perhaps,  be  identified  as  the  brain  stem 
reticular  formation,  the  most  caudal  part  of  which 
furnishes  crossed  and  direct  descending  pathways  to 
the  motor  neurons  in  the  spinal  cord. 

fe)  Cortical  theory.  The  famous  experiments  of  Fritsch 
&  Hitzig  in  1870  (51)  led  to  the  theory  of  the  cortical 
origin  of  generalized  epilepsy.  These  authors  demon- 
strated that  weak  electrical  stimulation  applied  to  the 
sigmoid  gyrus  in  the  dog  provoked  focal  movements, 
whereas  more  intense  or  more  prolonged  stimulation 
of  this  region  provoked  generalized  convulsions  more 
or  less  rapidly.  Today  we  would  consider  this  phe- 
nomenon as  a  secondary  (subsequent)  generalization 
and  consider  it  entirely  separate  from  seizures  gen- 
eralized from  the  start  (see  below).  At  that  date, 
however,  these  experiments  led  to  doubts  concerning 
the  subcortical  mechanism  of  epileptogenesis.  Thus 
Ferrier  in  1873  (47)  concluded,  "It  is  not  necessary 
to  assume  that  the  medulla  oblongata  is  the  primary 
seat  of  the  motor  disturbance  in  fits." 

Only  13  years  after  the  famous  experiments  of 
Fritsch  &  Hitzig,  Franck  &  Pitres  (49)  showed  that 
it  was  impossible  to  localize  an  epileptogenic  center" 
in  the  motor  cortex  only,  since  ablation  of  this  region 
did  not  stop  convulsions  provoked  by  its  stimulation. 
Thus  it  became  necessary  to  postulate  the  propagation 
of  epileptogenic  activity  from  the  point  stimulated  to 
the  whole  of  the  cerebral  cortex  or  to  subcortical 
structures  able  to  maintain  convulsions.  To  this  end, 
Unverricht  (1889)  announced  his  'law  of  irradiation', 
conceiving  an  'intracortical'  conduction  of  the  epi- 
leptic seizure  which,  starting  from  a  point  on  one 
hemisphere,  is  propagated  superficially  to  the  whole 
cortex.  Lewandoski  in  1907  (131)  defended  the  theory 
of  transcallosal  propagation,  as  did  Erickson  (43) 
much  later,  although  he  regularly  obtained  general- 
ized seizures  after  section  of  the  corpus  callosum. 
Since  these  fits  were  clonic  on  the  side  opposite  to 
the  stimulated  hemisphere  and  tonic  on  the  .same  side, 
Erickson  concluded  that  subcortical  structures  play 
an  accessory  part  in  the  propagation  of  the  epileptic 
discharge,  at  least  as  far  as  the  clonic  component  is 
concerned.  Karplus  (123)  on  the  contrary  supposed 


that  the  spread  of  a  discharge  localized  in  the  cortex 
takes  place  mainly  by  subcortical  pathways.  He  thus 
prepared  the  way  for  later  research  which  confirmed 
this  view,  as  we  shall  show  in  the  section  on  .seizures 
of  partial  epilepsy  secondarih-  generalized. 

In  these  theories  of  the  cortical  origin  of  generalized 
epilepsy  the  authors  assumed  that  convulsive  seizures 
depended  on  a  discharge  transmitted  from  the  motor 
cortex  of  both  hemispheres  to  the  spinal  neurons  by 
way  of  the  pyramidal  tracts.  This  concept  was  vig- 
orously attacked  by  Prus  in  1898  (162)  who  showed 
that  these  tracts  were  not  essential  for  seizure  pro- 
duction, von  Economo  &  Karplus  confirmed  this 
work  in  1910,  demonstrating  the  persistence  of  tonic- 
clonic  seizures  in  animals  after  bilateral  destruction 
of  the  pyramidal  tracts  and  the  pes  pedunculi.  The 
same  kind  of  experiments  were  undertaken  by  Mettler 
&  Mettler  in  1940  (141)  who  wrote,  "epileptiform 
.seizures  cannot  be  evoked  from  the  cortex  if  only  the 
pyramids  are  intact,  but  can  be  evoked  if  they  alone 
are  severed,"  a  conclusion  which  leaves  one  to  .suppose 
that  extrapyramidal  structures  and  pathways  play 
a  predominant  part  in  the  mechanism  of  generalized 
convulsions. 

Electrophysiological  experiments  soon  came  to 
confirm  the  results  of  these  ablation  experiments. 
Hoefer  &  Pool  (99)  showed  that,  during  a  seizure, 
spike-discharges  of  cortical  origin  are  intermittent  in 
pyramidal  pathways  but  continuous  in  extrapyram- 
idal pathways  situated  in  the  reticular  formation. 
Recently,  Zanchetti  &  Brookhart  (199)  have  demon- 
strated that  there  is  no  modification  in  pyramidal 
responsiveness  after  pentylenetetrazol  has  been  ad- 
ministered in  doses  large  enough  to  induce 
"spontaneous  convulsive  discharges".  Schlag  (172) 
obtained  similar  results  after  injections  of  physo- 
stigmine  or  acetylcholine.  The  two  authors  suggest 
that  these  convulsants  do  not  directly  affect  pyram- 
idal neurons  and  cortical  interneurons  but  act 
through  other  neuronal  structures,  notably  those  in 
the    reticular    formation. 

All  these  experiments  indicate  that,  although  corti- 
cal seizures  may  be  secondarily  generalized,  it  is 
unlikely  that  in  fits  that  are  generalized  from  the 
start  the  primary  origin  is  cortical  with  corticospinal 
propagation. 

c)  Eclectic  theory  nf  cortual-subcortical  mechanism  of  gen- 
eralized conviilsion.s.  Although  there  is  agreement  that 
generalized  convulsions  do  not  necessarily  depend  on 
the  cerebral  corte.x,  some  authors  consider  that  this 
applies  only   to   the   tonic   phase.   Among  these  one 


334 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


must  mention  Binswanger  (i8)  and  Wortis  &  Klenke 
(198}  who  obtained  only  tonic  seizures  by  mechanical 
or  electric  stimulation  of  the  pons  and  the  hypothal- 
amus, and  Ziehen  (201)  who  was  unable  to  obtain 
clonic  con\ulsions  after  cortical  ablation.  These  re- 
sults have  been  confirmed  by  Samaja  (171),  Prevost 
(161)  and  Bouche  (19).  This  difference  between  the 
sites  of  origin  of  clonic  and  tonic  convulsions  was 
elevated  into  a  law  by  Bechterew  (16).  Horsley  offered 
an  even  more  eclectic  opinion  when  he  wrote  in  1886 
(100),  "tonic  and  clonic  spasms  may  be  produced  by 
any  motor  center,  but  the  combination  and  sequence 
of  tonic-clonic  could  originate  only  from  the  cerebral 
motor  cortex."  Such  an  interpretation  however  is  not 
uni\ersally  accepted.  Bubnoff  &  Heidenhain  (23), 
Pollock  &  Davis  (159),  Pike  et  al.  (157),  Spiegel  (180), 
and  Marinesco  et  al.  (135)  insist  upon  the  fact  that 
clonic  as  well  as  tonic  convulsions  may  originate  e.x- 
clusiveh'  in  subcortical  structures. 

MEGH.^NISM    OF    THE    BIOELECTRIC    DISCH.^RGES    IN    GEN- 

ER.-^LiZED  EPILEPSY.  Certainly  the  most  striking  aspect 
of  generalized  seizures  recorded  from  the  cortex  is 
the  excessive  synchrony  of  the  elements  responsible 
for  each  wave  of  activity.  For  this  reason  the  epileptic 
seizure  has  .sometimes  been  called  a  'paroxysmal 
hypersynchrony'.  Actually,  the  synchrony  of  the 
components  is  always  imperfect  and  what  mainly 
characterizes  them  is  isorhythmicity  (48).  This  iso- 
rhythmicity,  as  well  as  the  approximate  synchrony,  is 
partly  explained  by  the  fact  that  different  cortical 
regions,  primarily  passive,  are  connected  with  one  or 
several  subcortical  foci  of  acti\it\'  which  act  as  their 
common  pace  maker  or  pace  makers.  We  know  that 
when  several  pace  makers  compete,  only  one  is  dom- 
inant, although  changeover  from  one  to  another  may 
take  place. 

This  community  and  this  unity  of  control  rep- 
resent what  one  might  call  the  external  factors  of 
isorhythmicity;  there  is  a  synchrony  or  at  least  a 
grouping  of  elements  when  the  controls  are  mediated 
by    fast    conducting    pathways. 

On  the  other  hand,  the  mechanism  of  internal 
synchronization,  which  engenders  and  organizes  the 
convulsive  subcortical  pace  makers  of  the  passive 
cortical  areas,  is  much  more  complicated,  depending 
on  many  intricate  factors.  Here  interactions  between 
neighboring  neurons  of  the  same  type  may  arise 
either  from  synaptic  connections  or  from  reciprocal 
field  effects  at  a  short  distance.  The  seizure  occurs 
when  these  interactions  become  unusually  important 
and  especially  when  they  thus  create  the  conditions 


for  explosive  autorecruitment.  This  may  come  from 
different  causes,  according  to  Fessard  (48) :  lowering 
of  the  excitability  threshold  of  neurons,  failure  of 
inhibitory  mechanisms,  structural  arrangements  favor- 
able to  synaptic  or  ephaptic  interactions,  alterations 
of  the  recovery  cycles  so  that  those  of  a  whole  popula- 
tion of  neurons  come  to  have  more  similar  perio- 
dicities, etc.  Even  chance  can  be  invoked,  for  if  the 
other  conditions  are  favorable,  a  fortuitous  and  ini- 
tially restricted  synchrony  may  hiring  on  synchroniza- 
tion of  excitable  elements  of  a  larger  population  of 
neurons  as  a  result  of  intense  interactions  which  will 
be  powerful  in  proportion  to  the  number  of  units  al- 
ready recruited.  The  fact  that  the  synchrony,  whatever 
its  cause,  results  in  wider  synchronization  is  the  basic 
principle  of  the  paroxysmal  character  of  seizures. 

The  generalized  nature  of  the  seizure  discharge 
which  accompanies  tonic-clonic  conxulsions  was 
demonstrated  by  workers  who  studied  its  distribution 
in  the  brain  of  animals.  Thus  Jung  (116)  by  using 
electroshock,  and  Gastaut  &  Hunter  (66)  and  Starzl 
('/  al.  (184)  by  injecting  pent\lenetetrazol  recorded 
such  a  discharge  from  the  whole  of  the  cerebellar  and 
cerebral  cortex  (i.so-  and  allocortex)  and  all  the  sub- 
cortical structures  from  the  caudate  nucleus  to  the 
mesencephalon.  Jung  observed  that  electrical  stimu- 
lation, which  was  insufficient  to  provoke  a  generalized 
fit,  caused  the  discharge  to  appear  in  subcortical 
(thalamic  and  subthalamic)  structures  and  the  allo- 
cortex (Amnion's  horn)  and  spared  only  the  isocortex. 
These  results  agree  well  with  those  of  Gastaut  & 
Hunter  (66)  who  observed  that  following  an  injection 
of  pentylenetetrazol,  bisynchronous  discharges  appear 
first    in    the   diencephalon.' 

Thus  one  may  suppose  that  generalized  discharges 
originate  in  diencephalic  structures,  whence  they 
irradiate  to  the  whole  of  the  brain,  a  hypothesis  which 
is  confirmed  by  direct  stimulation  of  the  median 
diencephalon. 

Electrical  stimulation  of  the  nonspecific  thalamic 
structures  at  low  frequency  provokes  a  'recruiting' 
response  (38-40,  106,  109,  183).  Such  responses,  when 

'  These  results  do  not  however  agree  with  those  of  Starzl 
et  al.  (184)  who  found  that  a  convulsant  dose  of  pentylenetet- 
razol pro\oked  first  a  cortical  discharge  which  was  secondarily 
'driven'  to  the  subcortical  structures  by  projection  fibers. 
Starzl  et  at.  even  concluded  that,  in  the  animal  with  an  en- 
tirely' isolated  corte.x,  the  convulsant  dose  of  pentylenetetrazol 
caused  a  generalized  cortical  discharge  without  any  response 
in  the  diencephalon.  These  are  obviously  disturbing  differences 
which  are  difficult  to  reconcile,  but  may  be  due  to  differences 
in  technique. 


THE    P.HVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


335 


evoked  by  mid-line  stimulation,  are  widely  distributed 
over  the  two  hemispheres  and  are  synchronous  and 
symmetrical  on  the  two  sides;  their  generalized  dis- 
tribution thus  resembles  that  of  a  grand  mal  seizure. 

Chemical  stimulation  of  the  diencephalic  brain 
stem  produces  generalized  electrical  discharges. 
Murphy  &  Geilhorn  (148)  obtained  generalized  dis- 
charges by  injecting  strychnine  into  the  hypothal- 
amus, and  recently  Ralston  &  Ajmone-Marsan  (163) 
provoked  bursts  of  bisynchronous  con\ulsant  waves, 
predominantly  frontal,  by  injecting  penicillin  into 
the  thalamic  reticular  formation  in  the  mid-line. 
These  bursts  could  \)e  precipitated  by  electrical  stim- 
ulation of  the  thalamus  and  appear  in  the  same  terri- 
tory as  the  'recruiting'  response.  The  authors  indeed 
believe  that  propagation  takes  place  along  the  non- 
specific thalamocortical  pathways  responsible  for  the 
recruiting  response. 

It  is  remarkable  that  generalized  cortical  discharges 
provoked  by  stimulation  of  the  thalamic  reticular 
formation  do  not  persist  after  cessation  of  stimulation 
and  are  never  accompanied  by  convulsions,  and  it  is 
even  more  remarkable  that  generalized  convulsions 
may  be  observed  in  the  absence  of  all  cortical  electrical 
discharge.  This  is  the  case  notably  in  the  tonic  seizures 
which  are  seen  in  certain  forms  of  syncope  and  which 
appear  in  the  EEG  as  total  electrical  silence  instead 
of  a  generalized  seizure  discharge,  as  shown  by 
Gastaut  &  Fischer- Williams  (63). 

It  is  interesting  to  note  that  techniques  as  unlike 
each  other  as  those  which  have  been  applied  to  the 
study  of  the  mechanism  of  generalized  convulsions 
and  of  the  accompanying  bioelectric  discharges  should 
lead  to  similar  conclusions:  the  incrimination  of  the 
brain  stem  reticular  formation  in  the  origin  of  these 
phenomena,  the  caudal  part  for  the  convulsions  and 
the  rostral  part  for  the  bioelectric  discharges. 

These  conclusions  obviously  invalidate  postulation 
of  direct  relationships  between  the  discharge  of  corti- 
cal neurons  and  the  muscular  contractions,  since  both 
of  these  depend  upon  a  third  event,  namely  the  reticu- 
lar discharge.  For  the  same  reason,  no  significant 
relationships  between  the  convulsive  brain  waves  re- 
corded from  the  scalp  and  the  convulsive  movements 
observed  at  the  periphery  are  to  be  expected;  they  are 
separate  both  causally  and  temporally.  These  con- 
siderations are  further  emphasized  by  the  following 
three  observations. 

a)  The  onset  of  the  EEG  discharge  and  that  of  the 
tonic  phase  of  the  grand  mal  fit  may  ije  separated  by 
a  relatively  long  time  interval. 

b~)  The  electrical  seizure  discharge  may  occur  in- 


dependently of  any  tonic-clonic  manifestations  of 
grand  mal,  and  vice  versa.  Thus  EEG  discharges  of 
the  grand  mal  type  may  appear  alone  in  sleep  (8y\ 
whereas  tonic  seizures  secondary  to  acute  cerebral 
ischemia  are  not  accompanied  by  any  electrical  dis- 
charge whatsoever. 

f)  The  EEG  discharge  and  the  convulsions,  even 
when  they  begin  together,  do  not  necessarily  e\olve 
in  a  way  which  is,  so  to  speak,  superimposable.  Thus, 
simultaneous  EEG  and  electromyographic  recording 
show  some  correlation  during  the  clonic  phase  but 
none  during  the  tonic  phase  (5,   168). 

PETIT  M.\L  OF  MYOCLONIC  TYPE.  This  has  bccu  induced 
in  man  and  animals  by  all  the  measures  capable  of 
provoking  grand  mal.  M\-oclonic  jerks'-  occur  before 
the  convulsive  seizure  when  widespread  interference 
with  cerebral  function  acts  in  a  sufficiently  slow  and 
progressive  manner  (anoxia,  oxygen  intoxication, 
hypoglycemia,  injection  of  pentylenetetrazol,  picro- 
toxin,  chloralose,  bromide  of  camphor,  etc.).  One 
has  but  to  increase  the  interference  and  metabolic 
disturbance  slightly  in  order  to  witness  the  appear- 
ance of  a  grand  mal  seizure,  following  the  m\  oclonic 
jerks. 

However,  as  in  grand  mal,  focal  cerebral  lesions  do 
not  produce  the  bilateral  myoclonic  jerks  of  petit  mal 
unless  they  are  near  the  mid-line  or  unless  their  action 
is  facilitated  by  the  addition  of  a  mild  widespread 
activator,  for  example  a  subcon\-ulsant  dose  of 
pentylenetetrazol. 

When  the  disturbance  is  severe  enough  to  pro\ oke 
spontaneous  myoclonus  with  its  accompanying  mul- 
tiple spikes,  any  moderately  intense  sensory  stimula- 
tion, such  as  a  sound,  touch  or  flash  of  light,  pre- 
cipitates myoclonus  after  a  very  brief  latent  period. 
Examples  include  the  myoclonus  provoked  by  sound 
after  sodium  .santonin  poisoning  (189),  myoclonus 
evoked  by  touch  and  sound  after  ingestion  of  bromide 
of  camphor  (149),  and  the  multiple  spikes  and  waves 
and  myoclonus  produced  by  flicker,  sound  and  touch 
after  pentylenetetrazol  (52).  There  is  here  a  further 
analogy  between  experimental  myoclonic  jerks  and 

-Jerks  in  myoclonic  petit  mal  must  not  be  confused  with  the 
clonic  phase  of  generalized  convulsive  epilepsy.  The  confusion 
arises  mainly  out  of  etymology,  but  we  consider  it  a  source  of 
grave  error  and  the  subject  is  later  treated  in  greater  detail. 
.Suffice  it  to  say  here  that  these  jerks  are  positive  phenomena, 
related  to  an  actual  neuronal  discharge  in  the  central  nervous 
system,  whereas  the  clonic  phase  in  a  generalized  fit  is  a  nega- 
tive phenomenon  and  represents  the  momentary  interruption 
of  the  prolonged  tonic  discharge  of  grand  mal. 


336 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


those  of  clinical  petit  mal  which  are  well  known  to  be 
precipitated  by  sensory  stimulation,  especially  those 
that  are  unexpected  or  repeated,  for  example  the 
intermittent  photic  stimulation  at  1 5  flickers  per  sec. 
used  by  Walter  et  al.  (193). 

The  possibility  of  producing  myoclonus  at  will,  in 
either  animals  or  man,  by  the  combination  of  camphor 
and  touch,  or  of  pentylenetetrazol  and  photic  stiinu- 
lation  enabled  Muskens  and  later  Gastaut  to  stud\ 
its  mechanism  and  describe  the  followina;  character- 
istics. 

a)  Myoclonic  jerks  occur,  as  do  the  convulsions  of 
grand  mal,  even  in  the  rhombencephalic  animal. 
(Similarly,  bilateral  myoclonic  jerks,  either  sponta- 
neous or  provoked  by  noise  or  touch,  are  character- 
istically seen  in   pontine  anencephalics.) 

i)  The  multiple  spike  and  wave  of  myoclonus,  far 
from  being  limited  to  the  rolandic  cortex  or  even  to 
the  whole  of  the  cortex,  is  recorded  in  all  the  grey 
matter  of  the  brain,  right  into  the  mesencephalon. 

f)  In  myoclonus  provoked  by  sensory  stimulation, 
the  electrical  discharge  appears  in  the  mesencephalic 
formation  and  the  thalamus  before  Ijeing  projected 
to  the  cortex. 

cf)  A  discharge  with  the  same  cerebral  distribution 
and  in  every  other  way  comparable  to  myoclonic 
multiple  spikes  is  provoked  by  electrical  stimulation 
of  the  anterior  part  of  the  thalamic  nuclei  in  the  mid- 
line. 

i)  The  clinical  and  electrical  phenomena  of 
myoclonus,  as  in  the  case  of  grand  mal,  may  be  en- 
tirely independent  in  their  time  relationships;  thus 
multiple  spikes  and  waves  may  occur  without  jerks 
and  vice  versa. 

With  reference  to  propagation  of  the  discharge,  it 
has  been  shown  that  photic  stimulation  in  an  animal 
given  pentylenetetrazol  provokes,  quite  apart  from 
the  specific  geniculostriate  response,  a  discharge  in  the 
reticular  formation  of  the  brain  stem  together  with 
an  ascending  thalamocortical  discharge  and  a  de- 
scending reticulocerebellar  and  reticulospinal  dis- 
charge (55,  65,  68).  The  existence  of  reticulospinal 
irradiation  is  also  implicated  in  the  work  of  De  Hass 
et  al.  (37)  who  showed  that  clonic  responses  evoked 
by  afferent  stimulation  persist  in  the  decorticate  cat 
given  pentylenetetrazol.  Muskens  (149)  in  1926 
already  had  a  presentiment  of  this  kind  of  irradiation 
when  he  related  the  sensoclonic  phenomenon  to  "an 
influx  produced  in  a  reflex  way  in  the  region  of  the 
reticular  substance  in  the  pons  and  medulla." 

Thalamocortical  and  reticulocortical  irradiation, 
observed  bv  Gastaut  &   Hunter  (66)  and  confirmed 


by  Hunter  &  Ingvar  (loi),  was  further  demonstrated 
in  an  indirect  way  by  the  results  of  De  Hass  et  al. 
(37).  They  showed  that  the  presence  of  the  specific 
.sensory  cortex  was  not  necessary  for  obtaining  an 
irradiated  frontal  response  to  photic  or  auditory 
stimulation  in  the  cat  after  pentylenetetrazol  ad- 
ministration; this  obviou.sly  excludes  the  hvpothesis 
of  purely  corticocortical  conduction.  Reticulocortical 
propagation  was  also  demonstrated  \i\  Jasper  et  al. 
(108)  who  showed  electrographically  a)  that  the 
postdi-scharges  from  the  visual  cortex  do  not  irradiate 
to  other  cortical  regions  by  corticocortical  pathways 
and  certainly  not  to  the  frontocentral  region  where  the 
multiple  spikes,  irradiated  under  the  effect  of  photic 
stimulation,  are  recorded;  and  b')  that  a  parastriate 
postdischarge  projects  directly  to  the  intralaminar 
nuclei  which  project  in  turn  to  the  frontal  cortex. 
It  seems  therefore  that  the  concept  of  the  subcortical 
origin  of  petit  mal  iif  nnoclonic  type  is  well  founded. 

PETIT  M.AL  OF  '.ABSENCE'  TYPE.  This  is  the  onh'  varietv 
of  generalized  epilepsy  which  has  not  been  satisfacto- 
rily reproduced  experimentally.  The  various  measures 
causing  generalized  cerel^ral  disturbance,  which  so 
effectively  reproduce  grand  mal  and  myoclonus,  have 
never  provoked  in  the  nonanesthetized  animal  tran- 
sient loss  of  'consciousness'  comparable  to  the 
'absence'  of  petit  mal. 

It  is  very  easy  to  induce  brief  loss  of  consciousness 
\)\  means  of  focal  cerebral  disturbances  and  particu- 
larly by  limited  electrical  stimulation  of  many  differ- 
ent structures  with  indwelling  electrodes  (thalamus, 
hypothalamus,  sul^thalamus,  the  basal  and  limbic 
rhinencephalon,  etc.).  Loss  of  consciousness  in  these 
cases,  however,  is  accompanied  by  an  'arrest'  and 
'orientation'  reaction  with  postural  readjustment  and 
various  types  of  gesture,  which  are  much  more  sug- 
gestive of  psychomotor  attacks  than  the  'ab.sence'  of 
petit  mal.  One  must  therefore  conclude  that,  despite 
the  attempts  of  various  authors  and  notably  of  Hunter 
&  Jasper  (102),  of  Kaada  (i  19)  and  of  Ingvar  (103), 
petit  mal  'absence'  has  not  yet  been  definitely  re- 
produced in  animals.  The  same  holds  true  of  man 
according  to  Gastaut  &  Roger  (77). 

On  the  other  hand  the  bilateral  synchronous  3  cps 
spike-and-wave  discharge  has  been  reproduced  in 
animals  under  special  conditions.  All  the  authors, 
having  injected  pentylenetetrazol  or  other  con- 
vulsants  systemically  in  the  anesthetized  and  cranioto- 
mized  animal,  have  provoked  at  will  long-lasting, 
self-perpetuating  discharges  of  spike  and  wave  which 
are  generalized,  bilateral,  synchronous  and  symmeiri- 


THE    PHVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


337 


cal.  This  spike-and-vvave  pattern,  howexer,  repeats 
itself  at  intervals  which  are  very  variable  and  only 
exceptionally  around  3  cps.  At  best,  it  can  not  be  re- 
lated to  any  modification  in  alertness  of  the  anes- 
thetized animal.  If  the  effect  of  rhythmical  sensory 
stimuli  is  added  to  that  of  pentylenetetrazol,  it  is 
also  easy  to  induce  a  spike-and-wa\'c  discharge  main- 
tained at  the  frequency  of  stimulation,  for  example  at 
3  cps  continuing  for  as  long  as  desired  (66).  However, 
the  fact  that  the  rhythm  has  to  be  maintained  actively 
and  ceases  as  soon  as  stimulation  is  stopped  com- 
pletely disqualifies  the  phenomenon  from  being  con- 
sidered as  a  form  of  experimental  epilepsy.  The  same 
criticism  may  be  levelled  at  the  spike-and-wave  in  the 
isocortex  or  allocortex  which  can  be  evoked  with 
great  difficulty  by  rhythmical  electrical  stimulation  of 
the  mid-line  nuclei  of  the  thalamus  (12,  103,  109,  118). 

Recently  Ralston  &  Ajmone-Marsan  (163)  have 
produced  in  the  cat  EEG  patterns  which  are  very 
close  to  the  bilateral  synchronous  spike-and-wave 
discharge  of  petit  mal.  They  produced  a  discrete 
irritative  lesion  in  the  nonspecific  thalamic  system  by 
stereotaxic  injection  of  penicillin.  As  a  result,  fusiform 
bursts  of  slow  waves  developed  at  a  frequency  of  3.5  to 
5  cps,  were  of  great  amplitude  and  tended  to  appear 
synchronously  over  the  ipsilateral  hemisphere  when 
the  lesion  involved  the  intralaminar  nuclei  but  over 
the  two  hemispheres  when  the  lesion  was  in  the  mid- 
line. On  the  basis  of  the  topography  of  these  thalamic 
lesions  and  cortical  discharges,  and  of  the  identity 
between  these  discharges  and  those  produced  by 
thalamic  stimulation  (either  single-shock  stimulation 
provoking  'triggered'  spindles,  or  repetitive  stimula- 
tion provoking  a  recruiting  response),  the  authors 
conclude  that  these  discharges  are  'transmitted'  by 
the  nonspecific  thalamocortical  projection  system. 
With  a  sufficiently  severe  lesion  spikes  also  appear,  at 
first  in  the  thalamus  and  later  projected  to  the  cortex 
where  they  may  be  grouped  with  the  bursts  of  hyper- 
synchronous  waves;  they  may  thus  sometimes  consti- 
tute rhythmical  spike-and-wave  complexes.  From 
these  observations  the  authors  have  come  to  believe 
that  the  discharges  of  petit  mal  depend  on  stimulation 
of  the  nonspecific  thalamic  system  near  the  mid-line, 
but  that  different  systems  are  insolved  in  the  pro- 
duction of  the  spikes  and  of  the  slow  waves. 

The  fact  that  it  has  not  Ijeen  possible  to  reproduce 
petit  mal  'absences'  in  animals  has  not  prevented 
experimental  studies  on  man.  Thus  Spiegel  et  al. 
(182),  Williams  (197)  and  Kirikae  et  al.  (125)  have 
recorded  numerous  episodes  of  'absences'  simulta- 
neouslv  in  the  cortex  and  in  the  thalamus.  All  these 


authors  admit  that  spike-and-wa\e  discharge  takes 
place  in  the  two  structures  at  the  same  time,  unless  it 
occurs  first  in  the  thalamus;  the  British  and  Japanese 
authors  e\en  feel  that  the  slow  wa\e  in  the  complex  is 
essentially  thalamic  whereas  the  spike  represents  the 
cortical  element.  Thus  Williams  suggests  that  the 
paroxysm  begins  in  the  thalamus  with  a  rhythm  of 
slow  sinusoidal  waves  of  which  each  element  propa- 
gates itself  to  the  cortex,  there  to  fire  off  a  spike  which, 
in  its  turn  transmitted  to  the  thalamus,  pro\okes  there 
another  slow  wave  and  so  on.  Ha\  ne  li  al.  (98)  report 
contradictory  results,  for  the>  do  not  believe  that 
there  is  any  discharge  in  the  thalamus  during  petit 
mal  'absences'  with  bilateral  and  s\nchronous  spike- 
and-wave  in   the  cortex. 

Petit  mal  'absence'  also  differs  from  the  other  types 
of  generalized  epilepsy  in  its  electroclinical  correla- 
tions. Loss  of  consciousness  is  undoubtedly  associated 
with  the  spike-and-wave  discharge,  since  no  clinical 
petit  mal  seizure  occurs  without  this  particular  dis- 
charge. The  reverse  is  quite  possible,  however,  and 
discharges  are  frequently  recorded  without  clinical 
manifestations. 

Although  electroclinically  allied  to  the  other  vari- 
eties of  generalized  epilepsy,  it  must  be  admitted 
that,  from  the  experimental  point  of  view,  the  find- 
ings relating  to  petit  mal  "absences'  are  not  analogous 
to  those  in  grand  mal  and  myoclonus.  Perhaps  for 
this  reason,  there  is  no  agreement  on  the  mechanism 
of  'absence'  and  its  accompanying  EEG  pattern, 
Shimizu  et  al.  (177)  believe,  indeed,  that  the  petit 
mal  spike-and-wave  discharge  has  a  localized  cortical 
origin  and  that  it  is  rapidly  transmitted  to  the  whole 
of  the  cortex  of  both  hemispheres  by  means  of  cor- 
ticocortical  association  pathways,  chiefly  via  the 
corpus  callosum.  They  base  this  view  on  their  electro- 
thalamographic  findings  in  man,  and  especially  on 
the  following  results  of  animal  experiments:  o)  intra- 
carotid  injection  of  pentylenetetrazol,  which  carries 
it  to  the  ipsilateral  cortex,  provokes  a  bisynchronous 
spike-and-wa\-e  pattern  more  easily  and  more  often 
than  does  injection  via  a  vertebral  artery,  which 
would  take  it  to  the  central  grey  matter;  H)  intra- 
carotid  injection  provoked  only  unilateral  spike-and- 
wave  complexes  when  pentothal  had  been  injected 
into  the  other  carotid.  However  this  may  be,  it  must 
be  admitted  that  this  view  is  peculiar  to  the  Chicago 
school  and  that  the  majority  of  electroencephalog- 
raphers  accept  the  theory  of  a  diencephalic  pacemaker 
mechanism  in  petit  mal  'absences'  as  in  the  other 
types  of  generalized  epilepsy  (105).  Ingvar  (103)  does 


338 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


not  fully  support  cither  theory,  believins;  the  matter 
unsettled. 

Cohn  (30)  pointed  out  that  the  spike  component 
of  the  spike-wave  (spike-dome)  complex  in  man  is 
not  exactly  synchronous  over  the  whole  of  the  scalp, 
since  its  beginning,  measured  at  two  homologous 
points  on  the  two  hemispheres,  could  show  an 
asynchrony  of  the  magnitude  of  about  5  to  20  msec. 
Such  a  finding  appears  to  lead  to  conflict  with  the 
theory  of  Jasper  &  Droogleever-Fortuyn  (109)  who 
argued  that  the  cortical  discharge  in  petit  mal  de- 
pends on  the  activity  of  a  single  mid-line  pacemaker 
which  projects  synchronously  to  the  two  hemispheres 
at  once.  Despite  their  discordance,  one  may  reconcile 
the  observations  of  Cohn  and  of  Jasper  by  taking  into 
account  the  anatomical  relations  demonstrated  by 
Nauta  &  VVhitlock  (151).  These  authors  showed  that 
in  the  anteromedial  part  of  the  thalamus,  which  was 
the  region  stimulated  by  the  Montreal  workers,  there 
is  a  band  of  compact  fibers  from  the  brain  stem 
reticular  formation  going,  on  the  one  hand,  to  the 
subcortical  grey  matter  and,  on  the  other  hand,  to 
the  nucleus  reticularis  of  the  thalamus  which  in  turn 
projects  diffusely  over  the  isocortex.  These  anatomical 
facts  may  explain  how  Jasper  &  Droogleever-Fortuyn, 
stimulating  in  the  mid-line  a  tract  of  fibers  with  bi- 
lateral distribution,  were  able  to  obtain  a  bilateral 
and  synchronous  cortical  discharge,  whereas  the  spon- 
taneous spike-and-wave  discharge  recorded  by  Cohn, 
arising  in  a  central  but  bilateral  and  diffuse  pace 
maker  in  the  thalamus,  may  give  the  asynchrony  which 
he  observed.  It  is  relevant  at  this  point  to  recall  that  it 
was  in  the  lateral  part  of  the  thalamus  that  Williams 
(197)  recorded  the  start  of  the  petit  mal  discharge. 

IiUerpretdtioti  of  Experunental  Results 

In  the  previous  section  we  ha\e  limited  ourselves 
to  a  presentation  of  experimental  results  with  a  mini- 
mum of  interpretation.  In  this  section  we  shall  be 
expressing  personal  views  in  an  attempt  to  present  a 
more  general  unifying  conception  of  the  physiopa- 
thology  of  generalized  seizures.  In  the  first  part  we  con- 
sider the  nature  and  origin  of  the  neuronal  discharge 
responsible  for  a  generalized  seizure,  taking  grand 
mal  as  an  example.  In  the  second  we  shall  envisage 
the  causes  of  this  discharge.  Finally,  a  third  part  will 
be  devoted  to  a  study  of  the  mechanisms  by  which  this 
discharge  is  prematurely  interrupted  or  rhythmically 
inhibited,  and  which  are  responsible  for  the  two  vari- 
eties of  petit  mal  (myoclonic  and  'absence'  types). 
The  final  aim  of  this  section  will  be  achieved  when  we 


have  demonstrated  the  existence  of  a  similar  mecha- 
nism in  all  three  main  varieties  of  generalized  epilepsy. 

ORIGI.N',  N.\TURE  .AND  PROP.AG.ATION  OF  NERVOUS  .\C- 
TIVHY'     RESPONSIBLE     FOR     GENERALIZED     GRAND     MAL 

SEIZURE.  If  we  envisage  generalized  grand  mal  epilepsy 
as  a  group  of  clinical  and  electrical  phenomena, 
necessarily  in\ol\ing  convulsions,  as.sociated  with  an 
EEC  discharge  of  generalized  hypersynchronous 
waves,  the  results  which  we  have  reviewed  in  the  pre- 
ceding section  lead  us  to  relate  it  to  a  reticular  dis- 
charge propagated  toward  the  cortex,  resulting  in  the 
EEG  manifestations,  and  toward  the  periphery,  in- 
ducing the  convulsions. 

This  conception  of  generalized  epilepsy  is  however 
disputable  in  so  far  as  it  regards  the  two  phenomena 
as  necessarily  associated,  and  affords  them  equal 
importance,  whereas  the  two  can  be  dissociated  and 
only  one  of  them  corresponds  to  the  clinical  definition 
of  epilepsy.  One  mas  try  to  explain  grand  mal  epi- 
lepsy in  terms  of  a  hypersynchronous  discharge,  but 
one  can  not  postulate  the  existence  of  hypersvn- 
chronous  discharge  in  an  affection  which  (until  we 
know  more  about  it)  is  characterized  only  by  con- 
vulsions and  loss  of  consciousness. 

If  we  admit  that  the  clinical  and  EEG  manifesta- 
tions of  generalized  epilepsy  are  not  neces.sarily  linked 
and  that  the  former  are  of  greater  'medical'  interest, 
we  should  first  of  all  try  to  explain  these  clinical 
phenomena  and  afterward  search  for  the  factors  that 
link  them  to  the  hypersynchronous  discharges  by 
which  they  are  usually  accompanied.  We  shall  there- 
fore examine  the  experimental  conditions  which  pro- 
voke transient  generalized  convulsions,  whether  or 
not  they  are  reputed  to  be  'epileptic',  and  seek  to 
delineate  their  precise  physiopathological  mechanism. 
In  the  present  state  of  knowledge,  there  are  four  con- 
vulsive conditions  which  throw  light  on  the  problem. 

a)  C'onvulsions  with  loss  of  con.sciousne.ss,  char- 
acterized by  inten.se  contractions  resulting  in 
opisthotonus,  preceded  or  followed  by  one  or  two 
muscular  jerks,  are  precipitated  in  man  and  in  ani- 
mals by  all  forms  of  cerebral  anoxia  (anoxemic  anoxia 
from  insufficient  partial  pressure  of  oxygen,  toxic 
anoxia,  and  ischemic  anoxia  due  to  cardiac  arrest 
and  fall  in  arterial  pressure).  These  phenomena  have 
been  studied  electrophysiologically  in  animals  by 
Noell  &  Dombrowsky  (152),  Ward  (194),  Ward  & 
Wheatley  (195),  Ajmone-Marson  &  Fuortes  (4)  and 
Gastaut  et  al.  (70).  The  concordant  results  of  these 
authors  may  be  summarized  as  follows. 

/)   During  acute   anoxia,   depression   of  electrical 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


339 


activity  extends  proSiressi\elv  from  the  telencephalon 
to  the  diencephalon,  and  then  to  the  mesencephalon 
and  the  metencephalon,  durintj  which  time  the  most 
caudal  structures,  notably  the  reticular  formation 
(in  the  pons,  medulla  and  the  spinal  cord)  de\elop 
or  continue  to  show  considerable  electrical  activit\'. 

1')  Anoxic  convulsions  are  no  longer  seen  after  the 
bulbar  part  of  the  reticular  formation  has  been  de- 
stroyed by  diathermy  (194).  One  must  therefore  con- 
clude that  anoxic  seizures  depend  on  the  activity  of 
the  caudal  reticular  formation  when  no  longer  under 
the  control  of  the  higher  nervous  structures  (fig.   i). 

ft)  Convulsions  without  loss  of  consciousness,  char- 
acterized by  intense  contractions  in  opisthotonus, 
preceded  and  followed  by  clonic  jerks,  are  provoked  in 
man  and  animals  by  the  administration  of  strychnine 
or  other  poisons  (e.g.  nitrogen  mustards,  di- 
chlorodiphenyltrichloroethane).  These  convulsions 
have  been  studied  from  the  electrophysiological  point 
of  view  in  animals  by  Bremer  (21),  Markham  et  a/. 


(136),  Ruf  (169),  Marossero  &  Garrone  (137), 
Bremer  &  Bonnet  (22),  Johnson  (112)  and  Gastaut 
el  al.  (71).  These  studies  give  the  following  remarkably 
similar  results. 

/)  .Strychnine  convulsions  are  accompanied  by  a 
hypersynchronous  discharge  in  the  whole  of  the  retic- 
ular formation  of  the  spinal  cord  and  the  brain  stem 
but  excluding  the  intralaminar  and  mid-line  nuclei 
of  the  thalamus,  stimulation  of  which  provokes  the 
recruiting  response. 

-^)  This  reticular  di.scharge  secondarih  extends  to 
the  cerebellum.  Bremer  has  shown  that  the  discharges 
recorded  in  the  cerefiellar  cortex  are  evoked  by  those 
coming  from  the  reticular  formation  which  he  con- 
siders the  site  of  the  autorhythmic  tetanic  activity. 
This  reticular  discharge  also  extends  to  the  motor 
neurons  of  the  spinal  cord,  the  hypersynchronous 
activity  of  which  is  directly  responsible  for  the  con- 
vul.sions.  On  the  contrary,  it  does  not  extend  to  the 
cerebral  cortex  which  reacts  by  desynchronization. 


««w«W»-W»«W»« 


»wi  OXi  >(»"»'W 


FIG.  I.  Schematic  representation  of  tlie  mechanism  of  anoxic 
convulsions.  The  density  of  the  horizontal  lines  is  proportional 
to  the  damage  to  the  neurons  due  to  the  anoxia.  This  damage 
is  maximum  at  the  corticothalamic  level  where  the  bioelectric 
rhythms  are  abolished  also.  It  is  diminished  at  the  level  of  the 
hypothalamus  and  especially  of  the  mesencephalon,  where 
there  are  still  slow  rhythms.  It  is  not  present  in  the  reticular 
formation  of  the  bulb  where  the  electrical  activity  is  normal. 
It  must  thus  be  concluded  that  the  anoxic  convulsions  (repre- 
sented by  the  arrows)  depend  upon  the  normal  activity  of 
the  caudal  reticular  formation  when  it  is  no  longer  subject  to 
the  control  of  the  higher  nervous  centers. 


FIG.  2.  Schematic  representation  of  the  mechanism  of 
strychnine  convulsions.  The  cross-ruled  areas  of  the  brainstem 
are  those  where  the  hypersynchronous  discharge  of  the  strych- 
nine type  occurs.  The  thalamocortical  structures  are  completely 
spared  by  this  discharge  and  show  only  a  desychronization 
which  is  normal  when  there  is  an  intense  excitation  of  the 
reticular  formation.  It  must  thus  be  concluded  that  strychnine 
convulsions  result  from  a  caudal  reticular  discharge  without 
any  participation  of  telencephalic  structures  or  even  of  the 
thalamus. 


340 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOCJV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


jj)  The  neuronal  reactivity  of  this  desynchronized 
cortex  is  normal  or  diminished,  but  never  augmented 
(199).  Chang  (26)  also  found  that  strychnine  de- 
creased the  excitability  of  the  cortical  neurons.  One 
must  therefore  conclude  that  strychnine  convulsions 
result  in  a  caudal  reticular  discharge  without  any 
participation  in  the  structures  of  the  telencephalon  or 
even  of  the  thalamus  (fig.  2). 

c)  Intense  tonic  contraction  with  loss  of  conscious- 
ness preceded  by  a  few  isolated  clonic  jerks  and  fol- 
lowed by  a  phase  of  rhythmical  clonic  convulsions 
are  provoked  by  different  convulsants  (analeptics), 
notably  by  thujone,  beta-ethylbetamethylglutarimide 
(Megimide)  and  pentamethylenetetrazol  (Metrazol). 
An  electrophysiological  study  of  these  convulsions  in 
animals  has  been  made  by  Gastaut  &  Hunter  (65), 
Gastaut  et  al.  (71),  AJmone-Marsan  &  Marossero  (5) 
and  Starzl  et  al.  (184)  with  the  following  results. 

/}  During  these  convulsions'  a  hvpers\nchronous 
discharge  replaces  all  normal  activity  in  the  di-  and 
telencephalic  formations,  clearly  predominating  over 
the  cortex  and  in  the  thalamus.  This  discharge  de- 
creases in  importance  in  the  midbrain  tegmentum 
where  it  is  not  able  to  replace  local  spontaneous  ac- 
tivity. It  is  practically  absent  from  the  rhombencepha- 
lon and  the  spinal  cord  where  normal  or  increased 
spontaneous   rhythms   continue. 

2)  The  responsiveness  of  the  cortex  to  electrical 
stimulation  (as  shown  by  the  threshold  and  sensitivity 
of  the  corticospinal  neurons)  remains  unchanged  even 
when  pentylenetetrazol  is  used  in  sufficientlv  large 
quantities  to  induce  'spontaneous'  consulsive  dis- 
charges (199).  Assessing  neuronal  excitability  by  the 
chronaxic  method,  Chauchard  et  al.  (27)  demon- 
strated that  subconviilsant  doses  of  pentylenetetrazol 
depressed  excitabilitx'  of  the  cortex  while  increasing 
that  of  the  brain  stem  and  spinal  cord,  an  action  com- 
parable  to  that  of  anoxia. 

One  must  therefore  conclude  that  pent\lenetetra- 
zol-induced  convulsions  are  evoked  b\-  a  mechanism 
which  is  analogous  to  that  of  anoxic  seizures,  i.e.  a 
'liberation'  of  the  activity  of  the  caudal  reticular  for- 
mation because  the  overlying  nervous  structures  are 
functionally  e.xcluded,  having  been  in\aded  by  a 
discharge  (fig.  3). 

d)  Convulsions  have  been  pro\-oked  in  animals  bv 
the   administration   of  pentylenetetrazol    in   stronglv 

'  We  will  comider  in  this  paragraph  and  the  next  only  the 
tonic  phase  of  convulsions  provoked  by  analeptics.  The  rhyth- 
mic clonic  phase  which  follows  the  tonic  phase  depends  on 
the  effect  of  a  special  inhibitory  mechanism,  which  will  be  the 
object  of  a  specicil  study  later. 


strychninized  animals  (3,  5).  These  convulsions  are 
expressed  by: 

/)  hypersynchronous  di-  and  telencephalic  dis- 
charge of  the  pentslenetetrazol  type  and  by  a  reticular 
discharge  in  the  mesorhombencephalon  of  strychnine 
type,  these  discharges  developing  completely  inde- 
pendently; 

2)  tonic  spasm  of  purely  strychnine  type  not  bear- 
ing any  resemblance  to  pent\lenetetrazoI  convulsions 
or  any  relation  to  the  cortical  pentslenetetrazol  dis- 
charge. 

One  must  therefore  conclude  that  these  seizures 
result  from  a  caudal  reticular  discharge  of  strychnine 
nature,  without  the  participation  of  the  di-  and 
telencephalic  structures  acti\ated  by  pentylenetetra- 
zol (fig.  4). 

Comparing  these  different  mechanisms,  it  appears 
that  generalized  tonic  spasm  and  isolated  clonic  con- 
vulsions depend  exclusively  on  the  caudal  reticular 
formation  which  acts  on  the  effector  neurons,  and 
particularly  on  the  motor  neurons  of  the  spinal  cord, 
by  means  of  the  various  reticulospinal  and  vestibulo- 
spinal pathways  and  projections.  These  pathways, 
like  the  structures  from  which  they  come,  are  capable 
of  inhibiting  as  well  as  reinforcing  muscle  tone,  but 
not  in  the  same  proportion  since  only  the  medial  part 
of  the  caudal  reticular  formation  is  inhibitory,  whereas 
all  the  rest  of  the  reticular  formation  and  the  \'estibu- 
lar  formation  is  facilitatory  (133).  It  may  therefore  be 
supposed  that  the  inhibiting  action  is  less  efficacious 
than  the  facilitating  one  and  that  it  is  entirely  masked 
when  the  reticular  formation  is  activated  as  a  whole. 
These  views  are  consistent  with  the  findings  of  the 
classical  neurophysiologists  of  the  Sherrington  school 
for  when  the  portion  of  the  reticular  system,  inhibi- 
tory as  well  as  facilitatory,  lying  caudal  to  a  midbrain 
transection,  is  liberated  from  the  influence  of  higher 
centers,  a  state  of  decerebrate  rigidity  results  and  not 
one  of  hypotonia.  The  mode  of  activation  of  this 
caudal  reticular  formation  \aries,  howe\er,  for  during 
the  tonic  spasms  it  may  represent  either  a  positive 
phenomenon,  a  hypersynchronous  neuronal  discharge 
or  a  negati\e  phenomenon,  a  liberation  by  depression 
or  functional   exclusion  of  the  oxerlying  structures. 

The  lo.ss  of  consciousness  accompanying  the  con\ul- 
sions  would  .seem  to  depend  exclusively  on  the  rostral 
thalamic  reticular  formation  and  the  cortex.  In  man 
anoxic  seizures  (in  certain  syncopes)  and  pentylene- 
tetrazol seizures,  as  indeed  all  other  generalized 
epileptic  seizures,  are  accompanied  by  unconscious- 
ness related  to  functional  exclusion  of  the  thalamo- 
cortical system  which  is  either  depri\"ed  of  oxygen  or 
occupied    by    a    hypersynchronous    discharge.    Con- 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOI.OGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES  34 1 


FIG.  3.  Schematic  representation  of  the  mechanism  of  pentyl- 
enetetrazol (Metrazol)  con\'ulsions.  The  density  of  the  vertical 
lines  is  proportional  to  the  importance  of  the  hypersynchronous 
discharge  of  the  pentylenetetrazol  type.  This  discharge  is 
maximum  at  the  thalamocortical  level  and  it  diminishes  in 
the  mesencephalon  and  the  metencephalon  to  disappear  in 
the  caudal  reticular  formation  where  normal  electrical  activity 
persists.  It  must  thus  be  concluded  that  the  pentylenetetrazol 
convulsions  are  produced  by  the  same  mechanism  as  the 
anoxic,  i.e.  a  liberation'  of  the  activity  of  the  neurons  of  the 
caudal  reticular  formation  when  the  higher  nervous  centers 
are  invaded  by  a  discharge  which  results  in  their  functional 
exclusion. 


FIG.  4.  Schematic  representation  of  the  effect  provoked  by 
pentylenetetrazol  in  an  animal  already  heavily  strychninizcd. 
The  vertical  lines  represent  the  hypersynchronous  pentyl- 
enetetrazol discharge  at  the  thalamocortical  level,  while  the 
squares  represent  the  hypersynchronous  strychnine  discharge 
in  the  brain  stem.  It  can  be  seen  that  the  strychnine  tetanus 
which  is  present  at  this  time  depends  exclusively  on  the  dis- 
charge of  the  caudal  reticular  formation  without  any  involve- 
ment of  the  diencephalic  structures  activated  by  the  pentyl- 
enetetrazol. 


versely  strychnine  convulsions  (tetanus  or  raJDies 
spasms  and  tonic  cerebellar  fits  probably  depend  on 
the  same  mechanism)  do  not  involve  the  thalamo- 
cortical system  and  are  characterized  by  preservation 
of  consciousness. 

We  may  now  attempt  to  apply  these  hypotheses  to 
the  convulsions  of  grand  mal  epilepsy,  believing  that 
pentylenetetrazol-induced  seizures  are  the  only  ones 
which  faithfully  reproduce  spontaneous  epilepsy  in 
man  with  its  hypersynchronous  cortical  discharge  and 
its  well  differentiated  tonic  and  clonic  phases.  A 
grand  mal  seizure  seems  to  depend  on  a  thalamic 
discharge  which  involves  the  nonspecific  reticular 
structures  and  is  projected  to  the  cortex  in  what  may 
be  considered  a  generalized  recruiting  response  trans- 
mitted along  the  diffu.se  cortical  projection  pathways. 
Since  the  system  responsible  for  recruitment  is  also 
responsible  for  generalized  epileptic  discharges  (66, 
log,  163)  and,  since  it  also  seems  implicated  in  the 
production  of  i)ursts  of  barbiturate  'sleep'  (107,  143), 


one  is  tempted  to  compare  the  hypersynchronous  dis- 
charge of  generalized  epilepsy  with  a  sort  of  paroxys- 
mal 'sleep'  localized  to  the  thalamocortical  system 
and  provoking  a  functional  exclusion  of  this  system. 
This  functional  elimination  may  be  directly  responsi- 
ble for  the  loss  of  consciousness  and  indirectly  re- 
sponsible for  the  convulsions  b\  liberating  the 
underlying  reticular  structures.  Given  the  antago- 
nism which  exists  between  the  thalamic  and  the 
mesencephaiorhombencephalic  part  of  the  reticular 
formation,^  one  may  suppose  that  a  momentary 
depression  of  the  caudal  reticular  system  can,  under 

'  The  recruiting  as  well  as  the  augmenting  responses  evoked 
by  thalamic  stimulation  are  blocked  by  stimulation  of  the 
reticular  formation  (85,  147);  the  pyramidal  discharge  synchro- 
nous with  the  augmenting  response  is  suppressed  during 
reticular  formation  stimulation  (155).  Conversely,  the  re- 
cruiting response  is  enhanced  by  the  destruction  or  the  barbitu- 
rate depression  of  the  reticular  formation  (iii,  124),  as  well 
as   many   other   responses   induced   by  thalamic   stimulation. 


342 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


certain  circumstances,  favor  a  thalamocortical  dis- 
charge indirectly  responsible  for  the  seizure,  so 
explaining  the  part  that  sleep  plays  in  inducing 
generalized  seizures. 

Finally,  assuming  the  independence  of  these  two 
systems,  one  may  postulate  that  a  factor  which  can 
precipitate  thalamocortical  hypersynchrony  can 
equally  (and  independently)  precipitate  h>-persyn- 
chrony  of  the  reticular  formation.  In  this  way  there 
may  be  a  double  reticular  discharge  of  which  one,  the 
thalamic,  may  be  responsible  for  the  cortical  manifes- 
tations of  the  seizure  and  the  other,  the  mesencepha- 
lorhombencephalic,  for  its  peripheral  manifestations. 

One  therefore  arrives  at  the  following  conclusions. 
Generalized  grand  mal  epilepsy  is  related  to  a  sub- 
cortical mechanism  corresponding  first  to  a  paroxys- 
mal discharge  of  the  thalamic  reticular  system  trans- 
mitted to  the  cortex  by  the  diffuse  thalamocortical 
projection  pathways,  which  explains  the  loss  of 
consciousness.  This  discharge  results  in  functional 
exclusion  of  the  thalamocortical  formations,  thus 
liberating  'normal'  or  reinforced  activity  of  the  caudal 
reticular  system;  this  release,  by  putting  into  play 
the  tonicogenic  reticulospinal  system,  explains  the 
peripheral  convulsions. 

It  will  be  noticed  that  this  conception  is  closely  akin 
to  that  of  Hughlings  Jackson  who  related  epileptic 
seizures  not  only  to  ai^nornia!  neuronal  discharges 
but  to  consequent  liberation  of  other  parts  of  the 
brain. ° 

CAUSES  OF  RETICULAR  DISCHARGES,  THALAMIC  AND 
MESENCEPH.ALORHO.MBENCEPHALIC,       RESPONSIBLE       FOR 

c;r.\nd  m.al  EPILEPSY.  In  onlv  a  \ery  small  number  of 

*  In  1874  Hughlings  Jackson  wrote  (104):  "The  principle 
that  we  get  over-action  of  lower  centres  from  the  mere  re- 
moval of  the  higher  centres  has  very  important  applications. 
.  .  .  Strong  epileptic  discharges  paralyse  the  nervous  centre  (or 
much  of  it)  in  which  they  begin  or  through  which  they  spread.  " 
He  then  applied  this  generalization  "to  the  cases  where  the 
discharge  begins  in  the  highest  series.  There  is  loss  of  use  of 
that  series  after  a  discharge  beginning  in  it,  where  that  dis- 
charge has  been  excessive.  But  ob\'iously  violent  action  (mani- 
acal raving)  could  not  result  from  this  loss  of  use  (a  paralytic 
condition)  of  the  highest  centres.  That  accounts  only  for  loss 
of  consciousness.  .  .  .  That  is  only  the  patient's  negative  condi- 
tion, and  his  condition  is  duplex.  There  is  the  positive  element — 
the  mania — to  be  accounted  for.  My  opinion  is  that  the  mania 
is  the  result  of  over -action  (morbidly  increased  discharge,  but 
not  epileptic  discharge)  of  the  processes  just  below  those  which 
have  been  put  hors  de  combat."  VVe  realize  that  the  above 
remarks  do  not  refer  primarily  to  generalized  giand  mal 
epilepsy,  but  they  nevertheless  exemplify  some  aspects  of  the 
unrivalled  enlightenment  of  Hughlings  Jackson. 


epileptics  has  a  focal  or  diffuse  irritative  lesion  in  the 
reticular  formation  been  found  to  be  the  cause  of 
generalized  seizures.  However,  characteristic  lesions 
in  these  regions  have  been  found  in  certain  familial 
degenerative  epilepsies  (of  the  Unverricht-Lunsborg 
type). 

Since  there  are  no  demonstrable  reticular  lesions  in 
the  majority  of  subjects  with  grand  mal  seizures 
generalized  from  the  start  and  since  they  are  never 
present  in  ca.ses  of  experimental  generalized  epilepsy, 
the  discharge  responsible  for  these  seizures  no 
doubt  depends  on  a  functional  abnormality  of  these 
reticular  neurons.  It  is  pcssible  that  this  functional 
abnormality  depends  on  the  unique  anatomical 
arrangement  that  is  found  here;  numerous  afferent 
collaterals  (183)  arriving  from  many  different  parts 
of  the  peripheral  and  central  nervous  system  (fibers 
from  sensory  lemnisci,  the  special  senses,  the  cortex, 
subcortical  regions  and  cerebellum)  all  converge 
toward  common  reticular  elements  and  set  up  phe- 
nomena of  summation  (146).  With  this  spatiotem- 
poral  summation  on  neighboring  neurons  and,  pro- 
vided that  these  are  in  a  hyperexcitable  state  (either 
because  of  constitutional  factors  or  acquired  dis- 
orders), the  normal  inflow  of  nerve  impulses  in  the 
reticular  formation  may  cause  sufficient  synchronous 
cellular  potentials  to  build  up  an  effective  electrical 
stimulus.  This  stimulus  would  entail  a  discharge  of 
the  surrounding  hyperexcitable  cell  bodies  by  direct 
electrical  spread  independent  of  any  process  of 
synaptic  transmission  (ephaptic  phenomenon).  Once 
this  process  has  been  started,  the  discharge  would 
spread  like  an  avalanche  throughout  the  reticular 
formation  with  a  speed  of  the  same  order  as  that  of 
the  transmission  of  the  net  ve  impulse  from  neuron  to 
neuron.  This  theory,  formulated  by  Gastaut  (54),  is 
only  a  particular  application  of  the  general  hypothesis 
suggested  by  Moruzzi  in  1950  (145):  "A  normal 
neuron,  by  the  simple  fact  of  being  subjected  to  a 
bombardment  of  nervous  activity  at  high  frequency, 
can  enter  into  convulsive  state.  ...  It  is  the  ordinary 
inflow  of  ner\e  impulses  which  determine  the  fact 
that  a  neuron  passes  from  normal  activity  to  an 
'epileptic'  state.  .  .  .  Any  neuron  may  become  'epilep- 
tic,' simply  through  the  effect  of  bombardment  of 
nervous  activity." 

In  an  unstable  system  (such  as  an  organization  of 
hyperexcitable  neurons)  it  is  usually  an  external  force 
that  upsets  equilibrium.  It  is  not  surprising  therefore 
that  a  volley  of  impulses,  converging  on  a  center  whose 
state  of  tension  is  abnormally  raised,  supplies  the 
energy   necessary   to    build    up    a    hypersynchronous 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


343 


discharge.  This  conception  was  held  in  the  nineteenth 
century  by  pioneers  of  the  modern  study  of  epilepsy 
who  from  Hall  (96)  onwards  elaborated  the  bulbar 
reflex  theory  of  generalized  epilepsy  and  believed  that 
it  depends  on  a  "discharge  produced  in  a  reflex  way  in 
the  region  of  reticular  substance  of  the  pons  and 
medulla  ..."  (149)  under  the  effect  of  "an  exalted 
sensibility  and  excitability  of  the  medulla  oblongata" 
(174).  One  should  not  conclude,  however,  that 
generalized  epilepsy  is  merely  a  reflex  phenomenon 
and  speak  of  reflex  epilepsy;  this  unphysiological  term 
is  not  applicable  today  to  any  variety  of  epilepsy. 

The  hyperexcitable  state  of  the  neurons,  which  is  a 
factor  necessary  to  the  production  of  the  reticular  dis- 
charge, may  depend  on  the  existence  of  an  'irritative' 
cerebral  lesion  lying  in  the  neighborhood  or  even  at  a 
distance  from  the  brain  stem.  Johnson  &  Walker 
(i  13-115)  and  KopolofT  et  al.  (127)  have  shown  that 
epileptogenic  lesions  localized  in  one  hemisphere  are 
always  accompanied  by  a  diffuse  hyperexcitable  state 
of  the  neurons  manifested  by  a  general  lowering  of  the 
convulsant  threshold.  It  may  depend  equally  well  on 
a  functional  factor  as  yet  undetermined,  a  'humoral' 
or  'cerebral'  factor  acting  at  the  level  of  the  synapses, 
the  axons  or  the  cell  bodies,  and  responsible  for  an 
'epileptic  predisposition'  found  both  in  man  and  in 
animals.  This  epileptic  predisposition  and  hyperexcit- 
able neuronic  state  may  be  quantitatively  appreciated 
b>-  determining  the  convulsant  threshold  with  the 
photopentylenetetrazol  method  (52).  The  threshold 
is  low  in  patients  suffering  from  seizures  generalized 
from  the  start  and  in  those  whose  attacks  of  partial 
epilepsy  pass  very  easily  into  secondary  generaliza- 
tion. 

DURATION   AND  TERMINATION   OF  DISCHARGE   IN   GENER- 

.'^LizED  EPILEPSY.  The  duration  and  ending  of  a  grand 
mal  seizure  depend  on  a  dual  mechanism:  a  negative 
process  of  neuronal  exhaustion,  and  a  positi\e  process 
of  inhibition. 

The  first  of  these  mechanisms  which  may  firing 
about  the  cessation  of  the  seizure,  the  progressive 
fatigue  and  final  exhaustion  of  the  neurons,  is  at- 
tributed either  to  the  accumulation  of  acid  metabo- 
lites or  to  the  fact  that  the  reserves  necessary  for 
cellular  functioning  have  been  used  up,  or  to  both 
processes  at  once.  The  first  hypothesis,  "Ermiidung" 
in  the  sense  of  Verworn  (1900),  has  not  been  satis- 
factorily demonstrated,  for  although  the  pH  of  the 
motor  cortex  shows  a  tendency  toward  acidity  during 
the  fatigue  stage  of  a  faradic  seizure  (42),  it  changes 


toward  alkaline  values  at  the  end  of  a  pentylenetetra- 
zol-induced  seizure  of  generalized  epilepsy  (i  lo). 

On  the  other  hand  the  .second  hypothesis, 
"Erschopfung"  in  the  sense  of  \>r\vorn,  has  been 
largely  demonstrated.  Ruf  prolonged  a  pentylene- 
tetrazol seizure  for  30  min.  by  administering  oxygen  to 
an  experimental  animal  and  for  i  hr.  by  giving 
epinephrine  as  well  as  oxygen.  Davis  &  Remond  (35), 
using  a  polarographic  cathode  method  sensitive  to 
oxygen  concentration,  demonstrated  the  existence  of 
relative  hypoxia  in  the  cerebral  cortex  developing 
during  convulsive  activity.  \V'hatever  may  be  its 
intimate  nature,  the  part  played  by  neuronal  exhaus- 
tion in  the  electroclinical  manifestations  of  grand  mal 
seizures  is  supported  b\-  the  following  considerations. 

At  the  beginning  of  a  grand  mal  fit  the  EEG  dis- 
charge does  not  diminish  in  frequency,  for  there 
exists  an  initial  indefatigability.  This  however  may  be 
apparent  only  if,  as  Rosenblueth  &  Cannon  (168) 
believe,  hypersynchrony  is  still  incomplete  at  this 
moment  and  if  different  cortical  elements  are  respon- 
sible for  successive  convulsive  waves.  It  may,  however, 
be  real  if  the  neurons  enjoy  oxygen  pres.sures  at  the 
onset  of  the  seizure  distinctly  higher  than  those  which 
determine  its  extinction  and  if  in  addition  their 
hyperexcitability  is  so  intense  at  that  time  that  they 
could  discharge  with  very  low  ox\gen  pressures. 
Whatever  the  case  may  be,  the  EEG  discharge  of 
sustained  frequency,  characteristic  of  the  beginning 
of  the  grand  mal  seizure,  corresponds  to  a  discharge 
of  the  peripheral  motor  units  which  is  equally  sus- 
tained but  of  much  higher  frequency  and  which 
provokes  the  tetanus  at  the  beginning  of  the  tonic 
phase. 

Once  the  seizure  has  lasted  some  seconds,  progres- 
sive slowing  of  the  EEG  discharge  develops,  indicating 
increasing  length  of  the  functional  refractory  period 
of  the  thalamocortical  neurons  as  they  fatigue.  This 
increasing  state  of  fatigue  also  affects  the  reticulo- 
spinal neurons  and  thereby  converts  the  complete 
tetanus  into  an  incomplete  tetanus  which  imprints  a 
vibratory  character  on  the  last  part  of  the  tonic 
phase. 

At  a  certain  point  of  fatigue,  the  functional  refrac- 
tory period  has  become  so  lengthened  that  the  dis- 
charge is  interrupted  for  a  short  time.  This,  the  first 
period  of  extinction,  appears  in  the  EEG  as  an  inter- 
val of  electrical  silence  and  at  the  periphery  as  relaxa- 
tion of  the  tonic  phase  introducing  the  first  clonus. 
This  momentary  rest  permits  partial  recovery  of 
energy,  which  entails  a  recrudescence  of  the  dis- 
charge (first  group  of  spikes)  and  of  the  muscular 


344 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


contraction  (first  clonic  jerk).  A  further  period  of  ex- 
tinction then  follows,  longer  than  the  first,  and  is  itself 
foUovved  by  another  discharge.  The  cycle  continues  in 
this  way  throughout  the  whole  of  the  clonic  phase 
until  total  exhaustion,  which  is  characterized  by 
lasting  extinction  of  both  electrical  activity  and  con- 
vulsions.'^ 

Postictal  recovery  is  slow;  it  is  manifested  elec- 
trically by  the  appearance  of  abnormal  rhythms,  first 
delta  then  theta,  and  from  the  clinical  point  of  view 
by  coma  and  an  episode  of  gradually  clearing  con- 
fusion. 

This  simplified  picture  of  a  grand  mal  seizure  might 
lead  one  to  believe  that  the  fit  is  limited  to  a  cortical 
discharge  recorded  on  the  EEG  and  the  convulsions 
observed  clinically.  This  is  far  from  being  the  case, 
since  all  the  cerebral  neurons  discharge  at  the  same 
time  as  the  cortical  cells  recorded  on  the  EEG  and 
all  the  peripheral  effector  structures  are  activated 
at  the  same  time  as  the  skeletal  muscles.  Thus  the 
whole  of  the  autonomic  system  is  involved  in  a  grand 
mal  seizure,  but  its  efTects  are  masked  by  the  spectacu- 
lar nature  of  the  generalized  convulsions.  We  may 
recall,  for  example,  the  fact  that  the  smooth  muscu- 
lature is  brought  into  play,  in  the  pupils,  nipples  and 
viscera;  that  salivary,  sweat  and  vaginal  glands  are 
stimulated;  and  that  there  are  alterations  in  cardiac 
rhythm,  arterial  pressure  and  vasomotor  activity. 
This  widespread  action  is  easily  understood  since  the 
diffuse  projections,  which  radiate  out  from  the  brain 
stem  reticular  formation,  go  not  only  to  the  cortex 
but  to  all  the  grey  matter  of  the  brain,  and  because 
the  reticulospinal  pathways  connect  with  the  auto- 
nomic preganglionic  centers  in  the  brain  stem  and 
spinal  cord  just  as  they  do  with  the  somatomotor 
centers  in  these  same  regions. 

The  second  mechanism  involves  active  inhibition. 
It  may  be  supposed  that  the  rhythmical  interruption 
of  a  grand  mal  seizure  depends  not  only  on  neuronal 
exhaustion,  but  also  on  the  development  of  inter- 
mittent inhibition  in  '  suppressor'  structures. 

Expounding  this  theory  in  1949,  Jung  (116)  sug- 
gested that  the  inhibitory  structure  was  the  caudate 
nucleus,  for  it  was  from  there  that  he  recorded  large 
regular  slow  waves,  coinciding  with  the  episodes  of 
relaxation  in  the  clonic  movements  and  interposed 
between  the  fast  rhythms  recorded  from  the  cortex 
and  the  thalamus.  This  hypothesis  agrees  well  with  the 

"  However,  fatigue  is  probably  responsible  only  for  the 
progressive  slowing  of  the  discharge  and  not  for  the  rhythmical 
interruption  which  depends  exclusively  on  the  inhibitory 
mechanism  described  later. 


views  of  Dusser  de  Barenne  et  al.  (41)  who  believed 
that  inhibitory  neurons  situated  in  the  caudate 
nucleus  were  acted  upon  by  the  various  cortical 
'  suppressor'  zones,  and  that  they  were  efTective 
through  the  thalamus  and  a  corticocaudothalamo- 
cortical  circuit.  The  close  relationship  between  the 
theories  of  Jung  and  of  Dusser  de  Barenne  is  further 
demonstrated  by  the  fact  that  Gastaut  &  Hunter 
(66)  and  Starzl  et  al.  (184)  recorded  these  same  slow 
waves  in  the  intralaminar  and  mid-line  nuclei  of  the 
thalamus. 

From  these  experimental  results  one  may  postulate 
the  existence  of  a  thalamocaudate  inhilsitory  system' 
'  branched-off  in  a  side-chain'  from  the  nonspecific 
thalamocortical  projection  system,  a  system  which 
may  actively  inhibit  the  reticular  formation  of  the 
thalamus  as  well  as  that  of  the  caudal  brain  stem  and 
thus  prevent  the  discharge  of  cortical  spikes  at  the 
same  time  as  the  peripheral  contraction. 

In  other  words,  the  thalamocortical  discharge  of 
grand  mal  responsible  for  cortical  spikes  and  for 
reticular  '  release'  (with  its  consequent  tonic  phase) 
may  be  equally  responsible  for  putting  into  action  the 
inhibitory  system,  the  slow  wav-es  from  which  rhyth- 
mically interrupt  the  discharge  of  spikes.  The  slow 
wave  represents  not  a  convulsion  wave  but  a  veritable 
state  of  neuronal  depression  linked  to  a  phenomenon 
of  active  inhibition  (the  '  braking'  wave  of  Jung,  the 
phylactic  wave  of  Walter,  or  the  inhibitory  wave  of 
Gastaut). 

This  theory  explains  the  absence  of  the  true  clonic 
phase  in  anoxic  and  strychnine  seizures,  for  in  these 
the  telencephalon  which  incorporates  the  inhibitory 
system  is  functionally  depressed  or  not  actively 
brought  into  play.  It  explains  why  the  tonic  strychnine 
convulsions  can  be  interrupted  by  a  clonic  phase 
when  large  doses  of  pentylenetetrazol  are  injected  into 
a  slightly  strychninized  animal  (fig.  5).  It  also  ex- 
plains why  the  seizures  induced  by  pentylenetetrazol 
or  other  analeptics  in  the  diencephalic,  mesencephalic 

'  The  existence  of  definite  connections  between  the  head  of 
the  caudate  nucleus  and  the  nonspecific  formations  of  the 
thalamus  has  been  demonstrated  by  physiological  neuronog- 
raphy (176).  Histological  proof  of  these  connections  were 
given:  a)  by  Ranson  et  al.  (164,  165)  and  Papez  (154)  who 
showed  direct  connections  (in  both  directions)  between  the 
pallidum  and  the  anterior  ventral  nucleus  of  the  thalamus 
(the  former  receiving  fibers  from  the  caudate  and  the  latter  now 
incorporated  in  the  nonspecific  system  of  the  thalamus);  and 
A)  by  Stefens  &  Droogleever-Fortuyn  (185)  and  Nauta  & 
Whitlock  (151)  who  demonstrated  projections  between  the 
head  of  the  caudate  and  the  intralaminar  and  mid-line  thalamic 
nuclei. 


O.L.d 


THE    PHVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES  345 

■  ■    ■:^^y■  .■ 


strychnine    -iO    9«c. 


E  cardiazof  2  mm 


OPCg 

G£.5.g 

GLg 

ThaLg-    ■ 
F.PM.g.' 


F.RMd 


-• — ■■ — ■■^^"-4^^l;«l ,^ , 


'|.;/r..   '■ 

.  , 

, 

•■■■!i}'.^'-..,.;.. 

B 


■J.^lll Mjl'—Uf 


FIG.  5.  Experimental  evidence  for  a  thalamic  inhibitory  system  responsible  for  the  interruption  of 
the  tonic  convulsion  of  reticular  origin  and  for  the  tonic-clonic  course  of  generalized  epileptic 
seizures.  The  first  fi\e  tracings  are  cortical  (right  lateral  gyrus,  left  precruciate  gyrus,  left  ectosylvian 
gyrus,  left  lateral  gyrus  and  left  suprasylvian  gyrus).  The  sixth  tracing  is  from  the  lett  thalamus.  The 
last  three  tracings  are  reticular  (right  and  left  mesencephalic  reticular  formation,  below  the  plane 
of  the  red  nucleus,  and  the  bulbar  reticular  formation).  A.  The  cat  which  has  received  a  weak  dose 
of  strychnine  40  sec.  previously  begins  a  typical  hypersynchronous  discharge  after  an  auditory  stimu- 
lus which  provokes  an  evoked  potential  in  the  ectosylvian  gyrus,  in  the  thalamus  and  especially  in  the 
bulbar  reticular  formation.  Note  the  exclusively  reticular  level  of  the  hypersynchronous  strychnine 
discharge  and  its  bulbar  predominance,  while  all  the  rest  of  the  brain  shows  merely  desynchroniza- 
tion.  B.  When  the  action  of  the  strychnine  starts  to  show  a  decline,  so  that  the  reticular  formation 
discharge  is  of  decreased  amplitude  and  regularity  and  the  peripheral  tetanus  is  less  intense,  a 
strong  dose  of  pentylenetetrazol  is  given.  This  provokes  first  several  spike  discharges  appearing  inde- 
pendently in  the  bulbar  reticular  formation  and  in  all  the  rest  of  the  brain,  and  then  an  intense 
rhythmical  discharge  at  the  thalamocortical  as  well  as  the  mesencephalic  levels  but  sparing  the 
bulbar  reticular  formation  where  the  strychnine  discharge  persists.  This  episode  evidently  corresponds 
to  a  tonic  seizure  caused  by  excitation  of  the  bulbar  reticular  formation  by  the  strychnine  and  by 
'liberation"  of  this  structure  under  the  influence  of  the  pentylenetetrazol  discharge  which  results  in 
what  may  be  a  type  of  functional  exclusion  of  the  thalamocortical  level.  C.  Eventually  there  devel- 
ops in  the  thalamus  a  rhythm  of  slow  waves  of  large  amplitude  and  progressively  slower  frequency. 
During  each  of  these  slow  waves  there  is  electrical  silence  in  all  the  leads,  particularly  in  the  bulbar 
reticular  formation  where  each  slow  wave  interferes  with  the  hypersynchronous  discharge  of  the 
strychnine  type.  This  corresponds  to  active  inhibition,  since  the  tonic  seizure  stops  for  the  same 
time  during  each  interruption  of  the  bulbar  discharge,  thus  making  possible  the  rhythmic  relaxa- 
tions which  characterize  the  clonic  phase  of  the  seizure.  D.  Finally  all  parts  of  the  thalamocortical 
system  are  exhausted  (extinction),  while  the  strychnine  discharge  continues  unchanged  in  the  bulbar 
and  even  the  mesencephalic  reticular  formation. 


346 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSIOLOCY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


or  rhombencephalic  animal  should  only  be  tonic; 
they  may  be  accompanied  by  a  few  clonic  jerks  with 
brief  seizure  discharges,  but  there  is  never  a  clonic 
phase  for  this  only  represents  the  rhythmic  inhibition 
of  the  tonic  spasm.  Finally,  it  explains  the  erroneous 
interpretation  given  by  Ziehen  (200)  and  Bechterew 
(16)  who,  on  the  basis  of  the  results  of  decortication, 
related  the  tonic  component  in  grand  mal  to  brain 
stem  structures  and  the  clonic  component  to  the 
cerebral  cortex. 

One  may  conclude  therefore  that  neuronal  fatigue 
and  exhaustion  are  responsible  for  the  progressive 
slowing  of  the  cortical  and  muscular  discharge  in 
grand  mal  during  its  tonic  phase,  whereas  the  thal- 
amocaudate  inhil)itory  system  is  responsible  for  the 
periods  of  relaxation  in  the  clonic  pha.se  and  for  the 
episodes  of  cortical  electrical  silence  or  slow  '  ijraking' 
waves  which  are  the  EEG  accompaniment  of  the  re- 
laxation periods. 

MYOCLONUS  OF  PETIT  M.\L.  This  disorder  may  be  con- 
sidered as  a  miniature  or  extremely  short  grand  mal 
seizure  (54,  59,  149).  Numerous  arguments  may  be 
adduced  in  favor  of  this  concept. 

a)  The  etiology  is  often  the  same.  Myoclonus  is 
frequently  associated  with  a;rand  mal  fits,  and  precedes 
the  majority  of  spontaneous  grand  mal  attacks  (52, 
149,  170)  or  of  attacks  precipitated  by  pentylene- 
tetrazol (Feuillet).  In  most  cases  the  myoclonic  jerks 
which  precede  grand  mal  are  repeated  at  shorter  and 
shorter  intervals  until  their  fusion  constitutes  the  be- 
ginning of  the  tonic  phase  (Ribot,  Muskens). 

6)  Electrographically,  the  form,  frequency  and 
topography  of  the  discharges  are  the  same  in  grand 
mal  as  in  myoclonic  petit  mal;  the  multiple  spikes  of 
myoclonus  appear  like  a  i^urst  of  spikes  in  the  clonic 
phase  of  grand  mal  or,  even  more,  like  the  discharge 
just  at  the  onset  of  the  tonic  phase. 

c)  Clinically,  the  peripheral  manifestations  are 
similar  in  the  two  types,  generalized  increase  in 
muscle  tone  masking  the  fact  that  other  effectors  are 
brought  into  play.* 

d)  Finally,  experimental  studies  furnish  the  most 
important  arguments.  Myoclonus  is  provoked  experi- 
mentally by  the  same  procedures  as  grand  mal  fits 
and  is  accompanied  by  a  similar  thalamocortical  dis- 
charge.  This  lilDcrates   the  facilitating  reticulospinal 

"*  The  myoclonic  discharge  is  obviously  too  brief  to  cause 
glandular  secretion,  but  it  is  however  able  to  bring  about  a 
slight  alteration  in  arterial  pressure  (Morin  >&  Roger,  unpub- 
lished observations). 


system  responsible  for  the  momentary  tonic  reinforce- 
ment which  we  call  "myoclonus". 

The  one  feature  that  difTerentiates  myoclonus  from 
a  grand  mal  seizure  is  its  duration,  and  therefore  only 
the  abrupt  and  premature  ending  of  the  myoclonic 
discharge  remains  to  be  explained.  The  sustained 
frequency  testifies  to  the  fact  that  it  is  not  terminated 
by  exhaustion  and  that  a  process  of  active  inhibition, 
like  that  already  envisaged  in  regard  to  grand  mal 
fics,  is  probably  involved. 

Ii  may  i^e  concluded  that  patients  suffering  from 
myoclonic  petit  mal  possess  a  more  active  inhibitory 
system  than  those  with  grand  mal,  and  that  this 
system  is  thrown  into  action  from  the  start  of  the 
thalamocortical  discharge,  thus  bringing  about  an 
almost  immediate  interruption  of  the  seizure.  This  ex- 
plains why  the  generalized  muscular  contraction  is 
only  momentary  and  why  the  EEG  expression  is 
limited  to  a  few  spikes  which  are  isolated  or  followed 
by  one  or  several '  braking'  slow  waves. 

PETIT  M.AL  '.\bsence'.  Petit  mal  'absence'  may  be  in- 
terpreted on  the  basis  of  the  same  hypothesis  as 
myoclonic  petit  mal.  It  may  be  considered  as  a 
thalamic  discharge  occurring  in  a  subject  with  a  very 
effective  inhibitory  mechanism.  Because  of  this,  the 
discharge  is  inhibited  almost  immediately  after  it 
has  been  fired  and  a  slow  '  braking'  wave  appears  in 
the  thalamus  immediately  after  the  development  of 
a  single  spike.  The  rhythmic  repetition  of  the  phe- 
nomenon may  be  explained  on  the  basis  that  the 
termination  of  each  wave  of  inhibition  allows  the 
thalamic  discharge  to  reappear,  provoking  a  spike  and 
a  new  inhibitory  wave. 

The  spikes  and  slow  waves  are  transmitted  to  the 
cortex  by  the  system  of  diffuse  projection  and  furnish 
the  classical  spike-and-wave  recorded  on  the  EEG 
during  the  'absence'.  Relative  independence  may 
exist  between  the  two  mechanisms  generating  the 
spike  and  the  wave  so  that  they  function  separately 
for  a  certain  length  of  time;  this  may  explain  the 
numerous  cases  of  atypical  spike-and-wave,  and  par- 
ticularly those  cases  in  which  the  spike  disappears  and 
leaves  only  the  slow  waves  at  the  end  of  a  clinical 
'absence'.  This  independence  also  helps  to  explain 
the  observations  made  in  man  by  Williams  (197)  and 
in  animals  ijy  Ralston  &  Ajmone-Marsan  (163)  who 
dissociated  the  spike  and  the  slow  wave  in  the  thala- 
mus and  on  the  cortex. 

Most  of  the  features  of  petit  mal  and  notably  those 
which  distinguish  it  from,  or  even  oppose  it  to,  grand 
mal    may   be   interpreted    on    the    basis   of  the   pre- 


THE    I'HVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC   SEIZURES 


347 


dominance  in  petit  mal  ol  the  thalamocaudate  system 
from  which  are  generated  the  slow  waves  and  a  process 
of  active  inhibition. 

a)  The  loss  of  consciousness  can  be  related  to  the 
hypersynchronous  discharge  which  is  propagated 
from  the  thalamus  to  the  whole  of  the  brain  and  pre- 
vents normal  cerebral  functioning.  The  lack  of  con- 
vulsions may  depend  on  the  fact  that  the  reticular 
activation  is  rhythmically  inhibited  and  can  e.xpress 
itself  only  by  a  slight  muscular  contraction  with  each 
spike  of  the  spike-and-wave. 

/))  This  hypothesis  of  the  predominance  of  the  in- 
hibitory system  from  which  are  generated  the  slow 
waves  may  explain  why  petit  mal  is  seen  especially  in 
patients  with  a  well-marked  tendency  to  ictal  and 
interictal  slow  hypersynchronization.  Certain  '  alj- 
sences'  are  characterized  solely  by  a  discharge  of  slow 
waves.  There  is  also  a  prevalence  of  slow  rhythms  in 
iaetween  petit  mal  .seizures  (theta  rhythms,  delta 
rhvthms  in  the  frontal  and  occipital  regions,  and 
hypersynchronous  bursts  during  overbreathing). 

f)  This  same  hypothesis  may  explain  \vhy  the  '  ab- 
sences' are  frequently  precipitated  by  conditions 
which  favor  this  slow  hypersynchronization  (hy- 
perpnea,  sleep,  closure  of  the  eyes,  and  administra- 
tion of  pentylenetetrazol,  pentothal,  chlorpromazine, 
etc.).  These  synchronizing;  measures  depress  the 
mesorhombencephalic  reticular  formation  and  thus 
■  release'  the  thalamocortical  system  of  spindles,  the 
hypersynchronous  discharges  which  depend  on  this 
same  thalainocortical  system  and  the  accompanying 
'  braking'  slow  waves. 

(T)  The  relative  antagonism  between  the  rostral 
and  caudal  parts  of  the  reticular  formation  may  throw 
light  on  the  fact  that  certain  physiological  conditions 
(such  as  puberty)  or  therapeutic  agents  (such  as  the 
diones)  can  transforin  petit  mal '  absences'  into  grand 
mal  seizures.  Petit  mal  is  distinguished  from  grand 
mal  by  this  functional  predominance  of  thalamo- 
caudate inhibition,  so  that  the  hypersynchronous 
thalamic  discharge  is  prematurely  inhibited  and 
liberation  of  the  caudal  reticular  formation  is  pre- 
vented. One  has  but  to  suppose  that  endocrine  modi- 
fications or  certain  medications  selectively  depress 
the  inhibitory  system;  this  lessened  inhibition  mav 
explain  the  prolongation  of  the  hypersynchronous 
thalamic  discharge,  the  bulbar  'release'  and  the 
transformation  of  petit  mal  into  grand  mal. 

One  common  theoretical  basis  thus  may  explain 
the  three  varieties  of  generalized  epilepsy  which  have 
been  shown  by  empirical  observation  to  he  closely 


linked.  Grand  mal  and  petit  mal  in  their  pure  forms 
are  indeed  exceptional,  whereas  the  association, 
either  temporary  or  permanent,  of  two  or  three  forms 
is  the  general  rule.  This  theory  of  common  causalitv 
may  help  in  understanding  the  characteristics  of  the 
EEG  discharge  and  the  .somatic  manifestations  of  the 
three  types  of  generalized  epilepsy;  it  may  explain 
the  loss  of  consciousness  which  is  a  feature  of  grand 
mal  and  the  'absence'  of  petit  mal.  Myoclonic  petit 
mal  is  too  brief  to  interrupt  the  chain  of  psychological 
events  whose  temporal  dimensions  are  greater  than 
the  duration  of  the  seizure.  Indeed  one  cannot  en- 
visage the  receipt  and  transmission  of  messages,  their 
analysis  and  transformation  into  sensations,  ideas  or 
actions,  and  their  storage  in  the  form  of  memory,  at 
a  time  when  most  of  the  cerebral  neurons  are  col- 
lecti\-ely  occupied  in  discharging  simultaneouslv  and 
when  the  source  of  this  discharge  is  exactly  the  struc- 
ture whose  function  is  to  regulate  the  whole  of  cerebral 
activitv. 


PHYSIOP.'kTHOLOGV    OF    P.ARTI.AL    EPILEPSIES 

ExperimeiUal  Results 

Seizures  of  partial  epilepsy  have  been  reproduced 
in  animals  only  by  provoking  a  localized  cerebral  dis- 
turbance. Since  this  necessitates  opening  the  skull, 
the  method  cannot  be  applied  to  man.  All  experi- 
mental results  have  therefore  been  obtained  in  ani- 
mals, but  relevant  information  may  be  gathered  from 
patients  with  a  \'erified  epileptogenic  lesion. 

A  localized  experimental  cereljral  disturbance  can 
be  epileptogenic  either  directly  by  acting  on  the 
neurons  or  indirectly  by  causing  a  lesion  which  is 
later  epileptogenic.  In  the  first  case,  no  actual  lesion 
is  produced  in  the  brain;  the  epileptogenic  stimula- 
tion is  either  an  electric  current  applied  locally,  a 
source  of  heat  or  cold,  or  a  chemical  irritant  (strych- 
nine, penicillin,  carbachol,  creatine,  physostigmine, 
acetylcholine,  nicotine,  picrotoxin,  etc.).  On  the  con- 
trary, in  the  second  case,  the  cerebral  as.sault  does  not 
directly  precipitate  a  seizure  but  leads  to  localized 
cicatrization  which  is  responsible  for  the  irritation 
that  later  provokes  seizures.  Aluminum  hydroxide, 
acting  as  a  foreign  body  without  immediate  chem- 
ical action,  is  the  substance  commonly  used  to  pro- 
voke this  type  of  irritation.  In  both  cases,  the  cere- 
bral disorder  may  be  produced  in  the  cortex  or  in 
the  depths  of  the  brain  in  various  subcortical  struc- 
tures; in  both  cases,  it  gives  rise  to  seizures  which  may 


348 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


remain  partial  throughout  their  whole  duration  or 
which  may  become  generalized  only  after  a  certain 
time. 

EXPERIMENTAL    PARTIAL    EPILEPSY'    OF    CORTICAL    (iSO- 

cortical)  ORIGIN,  o)  Local  application  of  strychnine. 
When  strychnine  or  another  convulsant  is  applied  to 
the  isocortex,  it  provokes  clinical  and  electrical  mani- 
festations reminiscent  of  certain  seizures  of  partial 
epilepsy  in  man.  Only  those  clinical  manifestations 
which  involve  discharges  provoked  in  the  motor,  and 
notably  the  somatomotor,  areas  have  been  studied 
[(15)  and  numerous  more  recent  authors],  probably 
because  they  are  the  only  ones  which  can  be  demon- 
strated in  the  anesthetized  animal  under  the  special 
experimental  conditions  required. 

In  the  classical  'cortical  strychnine  clonus'  of  Bag- 
lioni  &  Magnini  (15),  convulsions  appear  in  the 
contralateral  musculature  a  few  seconds  after  the  ap- 
plication of  strychnine.  They  are  at  first  confined  to 
the  parts  corresponding  to  the  cortical  region  treated 
but  they  later  spread  to  other  muscular  groups  on  that 
side  of  the  body.  Contrary  to  the  generalized  epilep- 
sies already  discussed,  the  impulses  for  these  clonic 
movements  are  undouljtedly  transmitted  from  the 
somatomotor  cortex  to  the  spinal  motor  neurons  by 
way  of  the  pyramidal  tracts.  This  conclusion  arises 
out  of  the  following  experimental  results:  /)  convul- 
sive discharges  can  be  recorded  from  pyramidal  fibers 
synchronously  with  the  clonic  twitches  (2);  2)  both 
the  movements  and  the  pyramidal  discharges  con- 
tinue when  the  brain  stem  is  interrupted  at  midbrain 
levels,  leaving  only  the  pes  pedunculi  intact  (196). 

If  the  cutaneous  zones  corresponding  to  the  strych- 
ninized  region  are  stimulated,  these  convulsions  ap- 
pear sooner  and  are  intensified  and  rapidly  general- 
ized. This  "reflex'  reinforcement  of  the  cortical  epi- 
leptic process  was  discovered  by  Amantea  (8)  in  1921 ; 
it  is  lost  when  the  strychninized  cortical  focus  is 
destroyed. 

The  EEG  manifestations  of  the  local  application  of 
strychnine  consist  of  a  bioelectric  oscillation  of  great 
magnitude  (more  than  a  millivolt),  known  as  the 
"strychnine  spike',  which  is  repeated  at  more  or  less 
regular  intervals.  This  spike  does  not  remain  localized 
to  the  spot  on  which  the  strychnine  is  applied  but 
spreads  like  a  drop  of  oil  to  the  whole  of  the  cor- 
responding area  (for  example  to  the  whole  of  the 
somatomotor  area).  It  is  also  propagated  at  a  distance 
to  the  homologous  structures  of  the  opposite  hemi- 
sphere and  to  the  allied  subcortical  structures  (for 
example   to   the  \entrolateral   nucleus  of  the   thala- 


mus when  the  strychnine  is  applied  to  the  somato- 
motor region).  The  strychnine  spike  however  can 
develop  to  its  fullest  and  continue  to  be  repeated  even 
though  it  is  not  accompanied  by  these  phenomena  of 
propagation.  Indeed,  neuronal  isolation  of  a  cortical 
area  (that  is  to  say,  its  separation  from  neighboring 
cortical  areas  and  from  subcortical  centers)  does  not 
prevent  the  appearance  of  strychnine  spikes  on  local 
application. 

The  strychnine  discharge  has  been  very  fully  in- 
vestigated by  workers  in  basic  neurophysiology  [see 
bibliographies  (143,  146)]  because  it  is  so  easily  pro- 
voked and  so  easily  repeated.  From  these  studies,  and 
particularly  those  of  Jung  (117)  and  Moruzzi  (146) 
one  ma\'  draw  the  following  conclusions.  /)  The 
strychnine  spike  results  from  a  process  of  hyper- 
synchrony,  that  is  from  the  simultaneous  discharge  of 
the  great  majority  of  the  neurons  in  the  strychninized 
area,  a  hypersynchrony  which  probably  is  due  to 
ephaptic  (extrasynaptic)  interactions  between  the 
different  elements  which  are  put  into  play  by  elec- 
tric currents  conducted  through  the  intercellular 
spaces.  2)  Recorded  with  macroelectrodes,  the 
strychnine  spike  only  shows  its  slow  triphasic  (posi- 
tive, negative,  positive)  envelope,  which  corresponds 
no  doubt  to  slow  potentials  and  to  an  electrotonic 
spread  and  decremental  conduction  in  the  dendritic 
plexuses.  With  microelectrode  recording,  however, 
one  observes  in  addition  a  burst  of  very  rapid  spikes 
(400  to  1 ,000  cps)  which  begins  with  the  first  positive 
phase  and  ends  with  the  second  negatise  pha.se.  It  is 
this  burst  of  rapid  spikes,  which  is  transmitted  along 
the  axons  of  the  pyramidal  cells  (i)  to  the  spinal 
motor  neurons,  which  provokes  the  muscular  twitch. 

i)  Localized  electrical  stimulation.  The  electroenceph- 
alographic  effect  of  a  single  Isrief  electric  shock  is 
seen  as  a  \ariation  of  the  local  potential  which  differs 
little  from  the  strychnine  spike  except  that  it  is  di- 
phasic, at  first  negative  and  then  positive.  Using 
intracellular  microelectrodes,  Buser  &  Albe-Fessard 
(24)  were  aisle  to  record  this  slow  variation  of  po- 
tential at  actual  neuronal  level.  In  addition  the  micro- 
electrodes  record  the  burst  of  brief  spikes  (less  than  a 
millisecond)  at  high  frequency  (up  to  1,000  cps)  which 
accompanies  the  strychnine  discharge  and  which  is 
propagated  along  the  length  of  the  axons.  (The.se 
spikes  are  oljviouslv  positive  in  the  interior  of  the 
neurons  and  negative  in  their  neighborhood.) 

A  series  of  electrical  shocks  results  in  repetition  of 
the  above  phenomena  so  long  as  the  frecjuency  of 
stimuli  is  not  too  rapid.  Above  a  certain  frequency, 
the  discharge  appears  only  at  the  end  of  stimulation 


THE    PHVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


349 


under  the  form  of  a  self-sustained  electrographic  ac- 
tivity known  as  a  postdischarge. 

This  postdischarge  has  most  of  the  features  char- 
acteristic of  the  evoked  electrical  or  strychnine  po- 
tential :  /)  diffusion  to  the  whole  of  the  area  contain- 
ing the  stimulated  spot;  2)  propagation  to  the  contra- 
lateral homologous  area,  j)  subcortical  propagation 
to  allied  structures;  and  ^)  development  on  a  strip  of 
vascularized  but  neuronally  isolated  cortex.  On  the 
other  hand,  it  differs  in  that  it  has  the  peculiar  at- 
tribute of  being  self-sustained  and  of  continuing 
rhythmically  for  a  shorter  or  longer  time  after  the  end 
of  the  stimulation.  It  is  no  longer  a  single  bioelectrical 
oscillation  of  great  amplitude  repeated  at  variable 
intervals,  but  a  series  of  oscillations  slowing  pro- 
gressively and  soon  interrupted  by  intervals  of  elec- 
trical silence  of  which  the  last  represents  a  long  phase 
of  postictal  extinction. 

French  et  al.  (50)  have  observed  that  all  cortical 
regions  can  be  made  to  show  a  postdischarge  follow- 
ing supramaximal  electrical  stimulation,  but  that 
only  some  regions  show  a  postdischarge  from  stimu- 
lation which  is  only  just  above  threshold.  On  this 
basis  they  describe  zones  as  '  epileptogenic'  in  the  fol- 
lowing descending  scale:  the  motor  and  premotor  cor- 
tex (motor  area  for  the  face  and  the  hand),  and  the 
teletemporal  and  uncinate  cortex  being  most  sus- 
ceptible; next  the  posterior  insular  and  superior 
temporal  cortex;  and  after  that  the  parietal  cortex. 
On  the  other  hand  the  frontal  and  especially  the 
occipital  cortex  are  resistant  to  experimental  epilepsy. 

The  clinical  effects  of  electrical  stimulation  have 
been  studied  only  in  respect  of  the  somatomotor  region 
for  the  same  reason  as  in  the  case  of  strychnine  con- 
vulsions. The  potential  evoked  by  a  single  electric 
shock  is  accompanied  by  an  isolated  contralateral 
'  clonus'  identical  with  the  '  cortical  strychnine  clonus' 
of  Baglioni  &  Magnini  (15).  The  electrical  after  dis- 
charge is  accompanied  by  a  convulsive  attack  (which 
might  be  termed  a  motor  after  discharge)  involving 
the  appropriate  contralateral  part,  each  cortical 
oscillation  corresponding  to  a  clonic  jerk  and  to  a 
burst  of  high  frequency  activity  in  the  corticospinal 
pathways. 

As  in  the  case  of  strychnine,  one  can  facilitate  or 
prolong  the  clinical  and  EEG  effects  of  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  cortex  by  stimulating  the  appro- 
priate cutaneous  reflexogenic  areas  or  the  parts  of 
the  brain  that  project  to  that  particular  cortical  zone. 
In  this  way  subthreshold  stimulation  of  the  sensori- 
motor region  facilitates  the  provocation  of  a  seizure 
from  the  homologous  contralateral  area.  With  supra- 


maximal stimulation  applied  to  the  subcortical  white 
matter  after  removal  of  the  corresponding  somato- 
motor area,  it  is  even  possible  to  provoke  a  seizure  in 
the  homologous  opposite  region  (27). 

f)  Epileptogenic  cortical  lesions.  These  are  caused  by 
local  application  of  aluminum  hydroxide,  according 
to  the  technique  of  Kopeloff  et  al.  (127),  and  appear 
as  fibroglial  scars  developing  slowly  around  a  foreign 
body.  Attacks  of  partial  epilepsy  are  seen  4  to  1 2  wk. 
after  application  and  persist  for  several  years. 

The  clinical  effects  of  such  lesions  ha\e  been 
studied  most  frequently  when  they  were  located  in 
the  somatomotor  area  of  the  monkey.  These  take 
the  form  of  Jacksonian  seizures  beginning  in  one  limb 
or  the  face  on  the  contralateral  side  and  spreading 
progressively  (with  Jacksonian  march)  to  include  the 
rest  of  that  half  of  the  body.  Between  seizures,  there 
may  be  isolated  twitches  of  the  muscles  involved  in 
the  beginning  of  the  paroxysm  (epilepsia  partialis 
continua).  Peripheral  stimulation  of  all  kinds,  chiefly 
of  the  special  sense  organs  (e.g.  a  loud  and  continuous 
noise),  may  precipitate  or  reinforce  isolated  clonic 
jerks  and  may  even  fire  off  a  Jack.sonian  fit. 

\'ery  few  authors  have  had  the  curiosity  to  apply 
aluminum  cream  to  cortical  areas  other  than  the 
somatomotor.  Cure  &  Rasmussen  (34),  however,  ap- 
plied it  to  the  insula  of  monkeys  and  they  mention 
spontaneous  seizures  but  unfortunately  describe 
only  one,  characterized  by  a  bilateral  tonic -clonic 
spasm  without  any  localized  feature.  Kopeloff  et  al. 
(127)  applied  aluminum  cream  to  the  occipital, 
frontal,  middle  and  anteriortemporal  cortex  of  the 
monkey  without  producing  seizures  in  which  there 
was  any  detectable  motor  phenomenon.  Nor  did 
Gastaut  et  al.  (83)  observe  any  paroxysmal  motor 
effects  after  subpial  injection  of  aluminum  hydroxide 
in  the  cat  in  regions  corresponding  to  the  occipital 
lobe  and  to  the  tip  of  the  temporal  loije  and  the 
temporal  lobe  proper.  These  negative  findings  are 
very  important,  chiefly  in  so  far  as  they  show  that 
temporal  and  teletemporal  scars,  at  least  in  the 
monkey  and  the  cat,  do  not  provoke  'psychomotor' 
seizures,  sometimes  attributed  in  man  to  similarly 
placed  lesions. 

The  EEG  manifestations  resulting  from  experi- 
mental .scars  appear  in  the  form  of  slow  variations  of 
local  potential  in  a  sporadic  or  in  a  rhythmical  man- 
ner. The  sporadic  variations  are  analogous  to  those 
provoked  i^y  a  single  electric  shock  or  the  application 
of  strychnine,  since  they  appear  as  predominantly 
negati\e  polyphasic  variations  in  the  form  of  a  spike 
followed  b\'  a  single  slow  wave  or  a  spike-and-wave 


350 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


complex.  The  rhythmical  variations  are  in  every  way 
comparable  to  postdischarges  provoked  by  an  electric 
current :  that  is  to  say,  they  appear  as  rhythmical  dis- 
charges of  localized  spikes  whose  frequency  diminishes 
progressively  and  which  are  often  interrupted  by  slow 
waves  or  intervals  of  silence  before  they  come  to  an 
end. 

Recording  with  microelectrodes,  Thomas  et  al. 
(i88)  observed  in  addition  cellular  discharges  which 
were  rather  different  from  those  provoked  by  strych- 
nine or  a  single  electric  shock.  The  units  often  showed 
spontaneous  prolonged  bursts  of  activity,  usually  be- 
ginning with  a  high  frequency  train  of  impulses  (ap- 
proximately I, coo  cps)  followed  by  longer  bursts  at 
somewhat  lower  frequencies  (approximately  300  cps). 
Such  a  discharge  may  repeat  its  whole  cycle  inter- 
mittently or  settle  down  to  a  steady  train  of  impulses 
at  about  150  to  200  cps  which  may  be  kept  up  in- 
definitely. 

(T)  Propagation  0/  experimental  isocortieal  epileptie  dis- 
charges. The  use  of  recording  electrodes  at  a  distance 
from  the  stimulated  region  has  shown  that  all  epilep- 
tic discharges  are  propagated  locally  and  to  a  greater 
or  lesser  distance.  Local  propagation  proceeds  very 
slowly  (from  i  mm  per  sec.  to  i  mm  per  min.)  through 
a  multitude  of  fine  fibers  and  synapses,  often  arranged 
in  reverberating  circuits  which  constitute  the  fibrillary 
network  of  the  cortex.  Studying  local  propagation  by 
means  of  vector  recordings.  Green  &  Naquet  (93) 
came  to  the  conclusion  that  it  may  represent  extra- 
synaptic  spread  from  cell  to  cell  following  dendritic 
depolarization.  Sporadic  and  rhythmical  discharges 
are  propagated  at  a  distance  in  different  manners 
(160).  The  rhythmical  discharges  (the  'postdis- 
charges') are  propagated  much  more  widely  than 
are  the  sporadic  discharges;  it  is  the  former  type  only 
that  we  shall  be  considering  here,  for  it  alone  cor- 
responds to  the  propagation  of  an  epileptic  seizure. 
(The  sporadic  discharges  only  represent  the  '  inter- 
seizure'  irritative  manifestations,  and  a  knowledge  of 
them  is  not  indispensible  for  understanding  the  actual 
seizures.)  Numerous  authors  have  studied  the  propa- 
gation of  postdischarges  from  different  parts  of  the 
cerebral  cortex.  A  complete  bibliography  of  the 
numerous  works  devoted  to  these  cortical  postdis- 
charges can  be  found  in  Green  &  Naquet  (93).  We 
shall  give  only  a  brief  account'  of  subcortical  propa- 


gation of  postdischarges  engendered  in  different  parts 
of  the  cerebral  cortex,  neglecting  corticocortical 
propagation  which  takes  place  mainly  in  the  homolo- 
gous contralateral  region  by  means  of  commissural 
fibers.'" 

Frontal  postdischarges  are  propagated  chiefly  to 
the  brain  stem  reticular  formation  (tegmentum 
mesencephali,  hypothalamus  and  intralaminar  nuclei 
of  the  thalamus)  and  secondarily  to  the  caudate 
nucleus,  the  amygdala  and  the  hippocampus.  Cingu- 
lar  postdischarges  have  a  similar  but  less  marked 
propagation,  and  orbital  postdischarges  propagate 
particularly  to  the  amygdala  and  hippocampus. 

Postdischarges  in  the  motor  region  travel  chiefly  to 
the  brain  stem  reticular  formation,  the  septal  region 
and  the  corpus  striatum.  Temporal  postdi.scharges 
are  mainly  propagated  to  the  amygdala,  hippo- 
campus, septal  region,  subthalamus,  hypothalamus 
and  the  mesencephalic  reticularis,  and  secondarily 
to  the  corpus  striatum  and  the  pulvinar.  Occipital 
postdischarges  go  chiefly  to  the  thalamus  (pulvinar 
and  lateral  geniculate  body,  and  neighboring  intra- 
laminar nuclei)  and  secondarily  to  the  subthalamus 
and  the  reticular  formation. 

Thus  the  postdischarges  localized  in  the  cortex  are 
characterized  by  remarkably  important  subcortical 
propagation  which  nearly  always  involves  the  brain 
stem  reticular  formation  and  the  amygdalohippo- 
campal  system.  This  tendency  for  cortical  epileptic 
discharges  to  invade  subcortical  nonspecific  structures 
or  the  brain  stem  had  already  been  evidenced  by  the 
interseizure  sporadic  discharges.  Thus  von  Baum- 
garten  et  al.  (190)  demonstrated  the  reticular  influ- 
ence of  strychnine  spikes  and  potentials  evoked  by  a 
single  shock  in  the  rolandic  region;  this  was  mani- 
fested by  reinforcement,  or  conversely  by  inhibition 
of  the  spontaneous  discharges  of  single  reticular  units 
recorded  with  microelectrodes. 

EXPERIMENT.^L  P.'^RTI.AL  EPILEPSY   OF   RHINENCEPH.^LIC 

(allocortical)  ORIGIN.  We  shall  study  seizures 
caused  by  epileptogenic  measures  involving  not  only 
the  allocortex  but  all  the  rhinencephalon,  both  its 
cortical  and  nuclear  parts. 

a)  Implantation  of  in-dwelling  electrodes.  This  method 
has  permitted  the  study  of  seizures  of  partial  epilepsy 
provoked    by    electrical    stimulation    of   the    rhinen- 


'  This  summary  takes  account  of  the  works  of  Walker  & 
Johnson  (192),  Kaada  (119),  Ajmone-Marsan  &  StoU  (6), 
StoU  et  al.  (186),  Gastaut  et  al.  (72),  Jasper  el  al.  (108),  Segundo 
et  al.  (175),  French  el  al.  (50),  Poggio  el  al.  (158),  and  Creutz- 
feld(3i). 


'•"  Contralateral  homologous  conduction  takes  place  via  the 
corpus  callosuni  or  the  anterior  commissure  according  to  the 
site  of  the  lesion.  This  was  demonstrated  by  physiological 
neuronography  (140)  and  by  study  of  experimental  epilepto- 
genic scars  (127-129). 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF   EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


351 


cephalic  formations  (56,  72,  80,  81,  83,  122,  132,  150). 
The  clinical  manifestations  are  of  interest.  Stimulation 
of  the  hippocampus  or  gyrus  fornicatus  provokes  a 
simple  reaction  of  '  attention'  and  contralateral  'ori- 
entation' of  the  head  when  the  stimulus  is  of  weak 
intensity.  When  a  stronger  stimulus  is  applied,  it  pro- 
vokes more  complex  reactions  suggestive  of  anxietv, 
fear  or  anger.  In  every  case,  the  animal  shows  some 
lack  of  awareness  and  responds  little  or  not  at  all  to 
outside  influences.  This  impaired  responsiveness  con- 
trasts with  the  accompanying  portrayal  of  'arrest'  and 
'attention'  and  is  paradoxical  if  one  interprets  it  as 
the  expression  of  clouded  consciousness.  The  paradox, 
however,  disappears  if  one  thinks  that  it  reflects  ex- 
tremely concentrated  attention  on  an  abnormal  psy- 
chological event  created  by  the  stimulation,  perhaps 
an  illusion  or  a  hallucination. 

Stimulation  of  the  piriform  cortex,  or  the  under- 
lying amygdala,  provokes  complex  phenomena  in 
which  are  associated:  /)  contraversive  deviation  which 
may  or  may  not  be  accompanied  by  abnormal  tonic 
or  clonic  movements;  2)  complex  gestures  apparently 
reactive  to  abnormal  sensations  involving  the  bucco- 
facial  region  or  the  extremities  (licking  the  lips,  clear- 
ing the  throat  as  though  to  get  rid  of  a  foreign  body, 
or  lifting  and  shaking  a  paw);  j)  actions  with  a  feed- 
ing significance  (lapping,  mastication,  salivation  or 
deglutition);  and  ./)  changes  in  the  autonomic,  re- 
spiratory and  circulatory  spheres,  including  pupillary 
changes,  micturition  and  defecation. 

The  electroencephalographic  efifect  of  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  rhinencephalon  has  been  studied 
by  Gastaut  et  al.  (72,  80,  81,  84),  Gloor  (90),  and 
Feindel  &  Gloor  (46),  who  investigated  chiefly  the 
amygdala,  and  by  Kaada  (119,  121),  Creutzfeldt  & 
Meyer-Mickeleit  (32),  and  Andy  &  Akert  (10,  11) 
who  studied  the  hippocampus  particularly.  In  these 
studies  postdischarges  were  produced  which  involved 
the  structure  stimulated  (amygdala  or  Amnion's 
horn)  and  were  transmitted  to:  /)  the  homologous 
contralateral  region;  2)  allied  structures  such  as  the 
hypothalamus,  the  septum  and  the  anterodorsal 
thalamus;  3)  the  corpus  striatum  and  midbrain 
tegmentum;  4)  the  pyriform  cortex,  the  orbito- 
insulotemporal  cortex  and  secondarily  the  anterior 
part  of  the  gyrus  cinguli;  and  5)  sometimes  even  to 
the  rest  of  the  isocortex.  There  is  considerable  diff'er- 
ence  of  opinion  among  authors  as  to  propagation  to 
the  isocortex,  which  according  to  some  is  predom- 
inantly to  the  frontal  regions  and  according  to  others 
to  the  occipital  regions. 


Propagation  to  these  structures  may  be  either  si- 
multaneous or  successive,  and  Gastaut  et  al.  (72,  80, 
83)  particularly  stress  the  fact  that  the  postdischarges 
are  erratic,  and  that  they  may  be  transmitted,  for 
example,  from  the  amygdala  to  the  temporal  and 
septal  regions,  then  to  the  posterior  hypothalamus  and 
from  there  to  the  frontal  cortex,  returning  again  to 
the  temporal  region. 

6)  Local  application  or  injection  of  aluminum  cream. 
Stereotaxic  techniques  have  made  it  possible  to  pro- 
duce epileptogenic  scars  in  the  same  limbic  or  basal 
rhinencephalic  structures.  The  experimental  results 
closely  resemi^le  those  of  electrical  stimulation  (81, 
83,  84). 

The  clinical  manifestations  are  typically  seizures 
which  occur  2  or  3  mo.  after  injection  of  aluminum 
hydroxide  into  the  amygdaloid  nucleus.  The  following 
description  of  seizures  in  cats  is  given  by  Naquet(i5o): 
"The  animal  suddenly  changes  its  attitude,  some- 
times tries  to  escape,  becomes  anxious,  immobile,  then 
sniflfs  violently,  especially  to  the  side  of  the  amvgdaloid 
scar;  at  the  same  time  one  notes  pupillary  dilatation, 
clonic  movements  of  the  homolateral  eyelids,  rapidly 
followed  by  facial  hemispasm  with  deviation  of  the 
head  to  the  opposite  side,  clonic  masticatory  move- 
ments and  salivation.  The  seizure  may  stop  at  this 
stage,  or  else  the  cat  lifts  its  anterior  contralateral  paw 
and  there  appear  clonic  movements  of  the  whole  of 
the  contralateral  side  of  the  body  followed  by  a  gen- 
eralized fit  with  urinary  incontinence.  A  '  confusional' 
state  with  loud  miaowing  follows  the  seizure.  In  some 
cases,  there  are  in  addition  various  types  of  seizures 
which  are  predominantly  'psychological'.  Suddenly 
the  animal  becomes  immobile,  its  pupils  dilate,  its  be- 
havior changes,  it  lifts  its  contralateral  paw  as  though 
to  defend  or  attack,  there  is  marked  piloerection  and 
it  bites  if  one  tries  to  touch  it.  This  seizure  lasts  20  to 
40  sec.  and  suddenly  the  animal  becomes  aflfectionate 
again.  Alternatively,  the  animal  suddenly  tries  to 
escape,  miaows  fiercely,  its  pupils  dilate  and  its  be- 
havior gives  the  impression  that  it  sees  or  hears  some- 
thing alarming.  This  seizure  terminates  rapidlv." 

The  electroencephalographic  manifestations  will 
now  be  described.  Between  seizures,  one  observes 
sporadic  discharges  of  slow  waves,  of  spikes  or  spike- 
and-wave  complexes  at  the  periphery  of  the  amygda- 
loid, hippocampal  or  septal  lesions,  which  are  trans- 
mitted to  one  or  several  of  the  following  regions: 
uncus,  insula,  tip  of  the  temporal  lobe,  temporal  lobe 
proper,  posterior  orbital  region  (78,  84,  167).  These 
discharges  may  be  on  the  same  or  the  opposite  side 
of  the  lesion  and  sometimes  may  even  predominate 


352 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


on  the  contralateral  side,  but  they  are  never  bilateral 
and  synchronous.  Independent  contralateral  dis- 
charges may  indicate  a  secondary  vascular  extension 
of  the  lesion  to  the  other  side  (69)  but  may  also  indi- 
cate a  functional  '  unleashing'  of  these  homologous 
contralateral  structures  which  have  acquired  an 
epileptogenic  potential  through  being  bombarded. 
For  these  reasons,  ablation  of  the  epileptogenic  focus 
on  the  side  of  the  lesion  does  not  necessarily  lead  to 
the  disappearance  of  the  contralateral  discharges 
which  mav  persist  for  several  months  after  operation 
(84). 

During  seizures,  the  discharges  show  a  great  variety 
of  forms:  /)  rhythmical  discharges  of  spikes  or  slow 
waves  observed  around  the  lesion,  propagated  to  the 
same  cortical  areas  as  the  interictal  discharges,  chiefly 
to  the  orbitoinsuloteletemporal  cortex;  2)  propaga- 
tion of  the  discharge  to  subcortical  structures,  chiefly 
the  septum,  the  hypothalamus  and  the  tegmentum 
mesencephali,  generally  accompanied  by  diffuse  cor- 
tical manifestations  like  desynchronization  or  slow 
hypersynchronization  occupying  all  or  part  of  one  or 
both  hemispheres  (78,  84,  167).  Sloan,  Ransohoff  & 
Pool  emphasize  the  bisynchronous  4  to  6  cps  ictal 
discharges  which  they  recorded  in  monkeys  with 
amygdaloid  scars. 

EXPERIMENTAL      PARTIAL      EPILEPSY      OF     SUBCORTICAL 

ORIGIN.  If  one  excludes  the  amygdala  and  septum 
which  have  been  linked  to  the  rhinencephalon,  few 
subcortical  structures  have  been  studied  from  the 
point  of  view  of  experimental  epilepsy.  Different  parts 
of  the  thalamus,  subthalamus  and  tegmentum  mesen- 
cephali have,  however,  received  indwelling  electrodes 
or  been  injected  with  aluminum  cream  (72,  73). 

The  clinical  manifestations  produced  by  limited 
electrical  stimulation  include  autonomic  and  devia- 
tional  phenomena  which  bear  only  a  distant  and  frag- 
mentary resemblance  to  the  seizures  provoked  by 
stimulation  of  the  basal  rhinencephalon.  The  scars 
from  aluminum  implantation  have  never  given  rise 
to  spontaneous  seizures,  probably  because  the  di- 
encephalic structures  have  a  high  convulsant  thresh- 
old. However,  injection  of  subliminal  doses  of  pentyl- 
enetetrazol in  cats  with  diencephalic  scars  have  always 
precipitated  seizures  very  similar  to  those  provoked 
from  the  basal  rhinencephalon.  This  led  Gastaut  & 
Roger  (78)  to  believe  that  at  least  some  of  the  aspects 
of  rhinencephalic  seizures  depend  on  the  fact  that 
allied  diencephalic  formations  are  brought  into  play. 

The  electroencephalographic  manifestations  are  of 


several  types.  With  hypothalamic,  subthalamic  and 
tegmental  epileptogenic  lesions  there  are  sporadic 
and  local  interictal  discharges,  transmitted  to  the 
orbitoinsulouncotemporal  region  which,  as  we  have 
already  seen,  is  involved  when  the  rhinencephalon 
discharges.  This  curious  observation  is  explained  by 
the  findings  of  physiological  neuronography  and  of 
histology  which  demonstrate  a  large  number  of  con- 
nections between  the  orbitoinsuloteletemporal  region 
on  the  one  hand  and  the  hypothalamus,  subthalamus 
and  tegmentum  mesencephali  on  the  other  (67). 

Irritative  lesions  of  other  subcortical  structures 
cause  discharges  in  other  parts  of  the  cerebral  cortex. 
Thus  lesions  of  the  lateral  (dorsal  and  posterior)  and 
of  the  posterior  nuclei  of  the  thalamus  produce  their 
effects  in  the  posterior  temporal  and  the  parietal  cor- 
tex, whereas  lesions  of  the  pulvinar,  the  lateral  genicu- 
late and  the  corresponding  region  of  the  nucleus 
reticularis  affect  the  occipital  cortex.  Lesions  of  the 
medial  geniculate,  the  suprageniculate  nucleus  and 
the  corresponding  region  of  the  nucleus  reticularis  act 
on  the  superior  temporal  cortex. 

EXPERIMENTAL  PARTIAL  EPILEPSY,  SECONDARILY  GEN- 
ERALIZED. All  partial  epilepsies  may  become  gener- 
alized whether  they  are  of  cortical  or  subcortical 
origin,  and  whether  caused  by  direct  chemical  or  elec- 
trical stimulation  or  resulting  indirectly  from  an  epi- 
leptogenic scar.  The  partial  epilepsy  which  has  been 
best  studied  from  the  point  of  view  of  generalization 
is  that  caused  by  localized  cortical  electrical  stimu- 
lation. Generalized  convulsions  develop  when  the 
strength  of  local  stimulation  passes  a  threshold  value 
wherever  this  cortical  stimulation  may  be,  even  after 
sagittal  section  of  the  telencephalon,  diencephalon 
and  mesencephalon  (181);  it  is  thus  certain  that  the 
subcortical  structures  extending  as  far  as  the  rhomben- 
cephalon are  responsible  for  the  generalization  of  the 
convulsions. 

A  study  of  epilepsy  of  the  Openshowski-Speranski 
variety  leads  to  the  same  conclusions.  Here  generalized 
seizures,  so  frequent  that  they  constitute  status  epilep- 
ticus,  are  provoked  by  refrigeration  of  a  small  part  of 
cerebral  cortex  on  one  side.  This  is  a  generalized 
epilepsy  which  is  at  first  partial,  for  immediate  abla- 
tion of  the  refrigerated  zone  abolishes  it,  but  the 
generalization  is  of  subcortical  origin  since  convul- 
sions (which  are  bilateral)  still  appear  after  ablation 
of  the  somatomotor  region  of  both  hemispheres  (45) 
and  after  section  of  the  corpus  callosum  (179). 

Subcortical  structures  influence  the  generalization 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGV    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


353 


of  somatomotor  or  occipital  strychnine  epilepsy  under 
the  facilitating  effect  of  a  bombardment  of  afferent 
'  influx'  coming  from  the  corresponding  sensory  areas 
[the  epilepsy  of  Amantea  (8)  and  of  Clementi  (28,  29), 
described  on  p.  355].  This  led  Moruzzi  to  write: 
"When,  in  the  photic  epilepsy  of  Clementi,  we  il- 
luminate the  retina,  we  not  only  send  nervous  im- 
pulses into  the  striate  area  which  has  been  strych- 
ninized,  but  at  the  same  time  we  activate  the  whole 
of  the  cerebral  cortex  through  the  ascending  reticular 
formation  of  the  brain  stem."  The  generalized  seizure 
that  follows  is  presumably  subcortical  since  subse- 
quent ablation  of  both  somatomotor  areas  does  not 
prevent  the  convulsions  from  developing  (9).  It  is 
therefore  most  likely  that  any  discharge  of  partial 
epilepsy,  once  it  is  of  sufficient  magnitude,  can  be 
transmitted  to  the  centrencephalic  structures  from 
the  thalamus  to  the  medulla,  and  from  there  be  gen- 
eralized to  the  rest  of  the  brain. 

There  is  supporting  EEG  evidence  for  these  conclu- 
sions. Jasper  el  al.  (108)  showed  that  the  majority 
of  cortical  postdischarges  are  transmitted  to  the 
reticular  formation  of  the  thalamus  and  brain  stem. 
French  et  al.  (50)  demonstrated  a  .subcortical  reticular 
mechanism  in  generalized  postdischarges  provoked  by 
localized  cortical  stimulation.  "Surface  regions  dis- 
playing the  characteristic  local  response  (persistent 
after  discharge)  seem  to  have  the  capacity  secondarily 
to  excite  certain  diffu.sely  projecting  subcortical  struc- 
tures (reticular  formation,  septal  region  and  amyg- 
dala) which  are  capable  of  disseminating  the  induced 
discharge  widely."  Finally,  in  microphysiological 
studies  in  strychnine  epilepsy,  von  Baumgarten  et  al. 
(190)  have  shown  that  each  strychnine  spike  developed 
in  the  motor  cortex  alters  the  spontaneous  activity  of 
the  neurons  of  the  reticular  formation  so  that  their 
activity  is  momentarily  reinforced;  this  effect  must 
play  a  large  part  in  the  phenomenon  of  generalization. 

EXPERIMENTAL  PARTIAL  EPILEPSY  WITH  ERRATIC  DIS- 
CHARGES. In  some  cases,  a  seizure  of  partial  epilepsy 
stops  as  suddenly  as  it  starts,  the  postictal  electrical 
silence  appearing  simultaneously  in  all  the  discharg- 
ing structures.  In  other  cases,  however,  the  discharge 
comes  to  an  end  in  one  formation  and  is  transmitted 
at  the  same  time  to  another  more  or  less  distant  part, 
thus  prolonging  the  seizure.  This  phenomenon  was 
first  described  by  McCulloch  &  Dusser  de  Barenne  in 
'935  C'39)  with  reference  to  electrical  postdis- 
charges in  animals  anesthetized  with  diallyl  bar- 
bituric acid.  Walker  &  Johnson  (192),  studying  the 


same  phenomenon,  showed  that  in  the  normal 
monkey  localized  postdischarges  stop  abruptly, 
whereas  in  the  monkey  with  an  experimental  epilepto- 
genic lesion  they  are  transmitted  froin  one  cortical 
region  to  another  and  continue  for  several  minutes. 
McCulloch  (138)  reinvestigated  the  question,  with 
seizures  provoked  by  chlorophenothane  (DDT)  and 
other  poisons  and  particularly  in  a  case  of  status 
epilepticus  in  a  monkey  with  an  experimental  frontal 
epileptogenic  lesion.  He  describes  how  the  epileptic 
discharge  would  appear  at  one  point  on  the  cortex, 
disappear  and  suddenly  reappear  at  an  unforeseen 
spot,  like  a  'jack-in-the-box'.  Gastaut  &  Roger  (78) 
studied  multiple  and  successive  cortical  seizures  fol- 
lowing stimulation  of  the  amygdaloid  nucleus.  They 
showed  that  these  'surprise'  discharges  do  not  really 
arise  independently  in  different  parts  of  the  cortex, 
but  that  they  represent  one  and  the  same  discharge 
transmitted  from  a  certain  point  on  the  cortex  to 
allied  subcortical  structures  and  from  there  to  other 
cortical  regions.  It  was  only  in  1953  that  Gastaut  et  al. 
(75,  76)  demonstrated  in  man  the  existence  of  multi- 
ple cortical  discharges  probably  corresponding  to  this 
same  mechanism  of  'erratic'  propagation.  Since 
then,  the  Marseilles  workers  have  constantly  empha- 
sized that  these  erratic  discharges  are  frequent  and 
especially  significant  in  so-called  'psychomotor' 
epilepsy. 

Anatomical  Studies 

Patients  with  partial  epilepsy  usually  harbor  con- 
spicuous organic  cerebral  lesions,  in  contrast  to  those 
cases  in  which  the  seizure  is  generalized  from  the 
start.  In  cases  of  partial  epilepsy  with  a  single  somato- 
motor or  sensory  symptom  related  to  the  pre-  or  post- 
rolandic,  occipital  or  superior  temporal  regions,  a 
lesion  in  that  particular  area  is  usually  demonstrable 
anatomically  as  well  as  electrographically.  The  most 
frequent  lesion  is  a  cicatrix  or  atrophy,  and  much  more 
rarely  a  neoplasm.  The  lesion  is  usually  superficial 
and  involves  only  the  cortex  locally  (a  corticomenin- 
geal  scar  or  localized  cortical  atrophy),  but  some- 
times it  goes  deeper  and  is  not  seen  on  inspection  of 
the  exterior  of  the  brain. 

In  polysymptomatic  partial  epilepsy,  however,  with 
the  sensory,  mental  and  motor  manifestations  of 
psychomotor  epilepsy,  true  focal  lesions  are  usually 
not  seen.  The  lesions  on  the  contrary  are  remarkably 
diffuse  in  these  patients.  The  most  frequent  lesion  is 
corticosubcortical   atrophy  with   more   or   less  well- 


354 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


marked  neuronal  degeneration  or  necrosis,  associated 
with  reactional  gliosis.  The  cerebral  atrophy  may  in- 
volve the  whole  of  one  hemisphere  and  sometimes  also 
the  contralateral  cerebellar  hemisphere,  but  it  gen- 
erally predominates  in  the  temporal  lobe  and  may  in- 
volve it  alone.  The  lesion  is  maximal  in  most  cases  in 
the  internal  aspect  of  the  temporal  lol)e  and  the  in- 
ferior surface  of  the  frontal  lobe  in  the  region  which 
comprises  the  anterior  part  of  the  hippocampal  gy- 
rus— including  the  uncus  and  amygdala — Ammon's 
horn,  the  temporal  tip,  the  insula  and  its  opercula,  the 
anterior  perforated  space  and  the  posterior  part  of  the 
orbital  convolutions.  This  is  a  region  which  is  fur- 
rowed from  front  to  back  by  the  rhinal  fissure  lined 
by  the  endorhinal  fissure,  and  which  Gastaut  pro- 
posed to  name  the  '  pararhinal  region'  for  this  has  the 
advantage  of  connoting  the  allopcriallocortical  (rhin- 
encephalic)  nature  of  the  parts  inxoKed. 

Phruiipatliogenesis  of  Partial  Epilepsies 

ORIGIN  ."KND  CAUSE  OF  NEURON.AL  DISCHARGE  IN  PARTI.\L 

EPILEPSY.  The  discharge  in  the  partial  epilepsies  gen- 
erally starts  in  the  immediate  neighborhood  of  the 
epileptogenic  lesion  where  the  neurons  show  hyper- 
excitability,  as  demonstrated  by  Walker  &  Johnson 
(192).  These  workers  found  that  around  an  alumi- 
num-produced scar  weaker  electric  stimulation  pro- 
duced a  postdischarge,  or  smaller  doses  of  pen- 
is lenetetrazol   were   recjuired    to   evoke    local  spikes. 

In  certain  cases,  however,  the  discharge  begins  at  a 
distance  from  the  epileptogenic  lesions,  either  in  al- 
lied structures  or  even  in  structures  which  are  entirely 
independent.  We  have  already  seen  that  an  experi- 
mental epileptogenic  lesion  in  the  right  amygdala  in 
a  cat  can  cause  ictal  discharges  in  the  left  amygdala, 
and  in  man  a  right-sided  temporal  epileptogenic 
lesion.  This  is  seen  in  the  experiments  of  Walker  & 
or  left  temporal  or  in  the  occipital  regions.  These  facts, 
emphasized  by  the  Marseilles  school,  may  be  ex- 
plained by  the  conception  that  neuronal  excitability 
is  heightened  at  a  distance  from  the  epileptogenic 
lesion.  This  is  seen  in  the  experiments  of  Walker  & 
Johnson  (192)  and  of  KopelofTc^  al.  (127)  who  demon- 
strated a  lowered  convulsant  threshold  in  cortical  or 
allied  subcortical  structures,  and  even  in  the  whole 
jjrain  in  animals  with  an  epileptogenic  scar. 

It  is  a  very  important  conception  that  neuronal 
excitability  may  be  intensified  remotely  from  the 
epileptogenic  lesion  in  structures  anatomically  allied 
to  the  lesion  but  not  themselves  showing  anv  organic 


alteration.  The  degree  of  excitability  may  indeed  be 
so  high  that,  under  the  influence  of  an  afferent  volley, 
the  allied  structure  may  discharge  as  well  as,  if  not 
more  intensely  than,  the  epileptogenic  focus  itself 
(61).  One  concludes  therefore  (53-55,  57)  that,  al- 
though the  existence  of  a  sporadic  spike  or  a  rh\  thmic 
discharge  in  an  EEG  or  a  corticogram  constitutes 
the  most  reliable  proof  of  a  local  epileptic  process,  it 
in  no  way  guarantees  that  the  epileptogenic  lesion  is 
seated  in  the  same  place.  Working  on  this  general 
principle,  Gastaut  &  Roger  (78)  demonstrated  the 
following  facts. 

o)  The  epileptogenic  lesion  may  or  may  not  coin- 
cide with  a  given  EEG  focus;  it  may  even  be  a  long 
way  off.  Gastaut  (82)  showed  that  a  large  number  of 
occipital  seizure  discharges  appear  in  patients  with 
an  anterior  temporalpararhinal  lesion,  while  Segundo 
et  al.  (175)  observed  true  electric  occipital  seizures  in 
the  monkey  following  postdischarges  induced  in  the 
amygdala. 

h)  The  existence  of  an  EEG  spike  focus  is  always 
a  valuable  criterion  for  localization  in  partial  epilepsy, 
but  only  as  a  physiological  argument  in  relation  to  the 
clinical  facts;  it  never  permits  one  to  incriminate  a 
lesion  of  the  underlying  cortex  directly  and  with 
certainty. 

c)  A  spike  focus  in  the  electrocorticogram  is  always 
a  useful  finding  for  the  neurosurgeon,  allowing  him  to 
judge  where  the  primary  epileptogenic  focus  probably 
lies  on  the  basis  of  anatomophysiological  reasoning.  It 
ne\'er  unfailingly  indicates  the  territory  to  be  resected 
nor  its  lioundaries;  the  surgeon  has  to  remove  the 
lesion  or  the  structure  insolved  and  not  just  the  spike- 
ijearing  area. 

(/)  The  existence  of  several,  concomitant  or  inde- 
pendent spike  foci  does  not  necessarily  signify  a  cor- 
responding number  of  lesions.  Also,  the  existence  of  a 
focus  of  bilateral  and  symmetrical  spikes,  concomitant 
or  independent,  does  not  necessarily  signify  a  bi- 
lateral lesion. 

<")  The  persistence  of  a  spike  focus  after  ablation  of 
an  epileptogenic  focus  does  not  necessarily  mean  that 
the  whole  or  a  part  of  the  lesion  persists  nor  that  a  new 
lesion  has  been  created  by  the  operation;  it  may  be 
that  the  local  perilesional  hyperexcitability  persists  or 
is  enhanced  for  a  shorter  or  longer  time.  In  the  same 
wav,  the  persistence  or  appearance  of  a  contralateral 
spike  focus  after  ablation  of  an  apparently  unilateral 
lesion  does  not  necessarily  imply  that  a  previously 
unobserved  contralateral  lesion  exists;  it  may  be  a 
matter  again  of  local  hyperexcitability  which  is  trans- 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGV    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


355 


milted  to  the  other  side.  This  was  reproduced  experi- 
mentally by  Gastaut  et  al.  (84).  The\  provoked  bi- 
lateral and  symmetrical  spike  foci  with  a  one-sided 
lesion  resulting  from  aluminum  scarring  and  then  ob- 
served that  the  contralateral  focus  persisted  after 
ablation  of  the  single  lesion. 

This  instability  and  variability  of  the  epileptic  dis- 
charge is  seen  even  in  patients  who  only  show  peri- 
lesional  discharges.  These  discharges  originate  at 
some  point  on  the  periphery  of  the  lesion;  when  the 
lesion  is  extensive  and  surrounded  Ijy  a  large  '  halo' 
of  neuronal  hyperexcitaljility,  the  discharges  may 
arise  in  different  seizures  at  places  far  removed  from 
each  other.  This  was  recorded  experimentally  by 
Roger  (167)  in  whose  experience  the  seizure  dis- 
charges around  a  single  ijut  extensive  lesion  involv- 
ing most  of  the  amygdaloid  nucleus  sometimes  began 
in  the  hippocampus  and  sometimes  in  the  ento- 
peduncular  nucleus  or  the  anterior  amygdaloid  zone 
or  the  lateral  amygdaloid  nucleus.  Pathological  hyper- 
excitability  maintained  around  and  at  a  distance  from 
epileptogenic  lesions  thus  plays  an  essential  part  in  the 
development  of  the  seizures  of  partial  epilepsy. 

Of  equivalent  or  greater  importance  is  the  part 
played  by  the  innate  hyperexcitability  of  certain 
regions  which  show  a  low  convulsant  threshold  and 
a  striking  epileptogenic  predisposition.  The  various 
authors  who  have  studied  these  local  difTerences  in 
the  convulsant  thresholds  have  come  to  the  following 
conclusions.  The  hippocampus  has  the  lowest  thresh- 
old of  excitability  of  all  the  cerebral  structures  so  far 
explored  (11,  25,  31,  86,  94,  119-121,  142).  The 
motor  cortex  has  the  next  lowest  threshold  (168),  es- 
pecially in  the  region  corresponding  to  motor  repre- 
sentation of  the  face  and  hand  (50).  In  order  of  de- 
creasing e.xcitability  there  follows  the  cingular  region, 
the  tip  of  the  temporal  lobe  and  the  uncinate  region 
with  the  underlying  amygdala,  the  first  temporal  con- 
volution and,  finally,  the  parietal  region.  The  frontal 
region  and  particularly  the  occipital  region  have  the 
highest  epileptogenic  threshold.  It  is  hardly  neces- 
sary to  stress  the  importance  of  these  findings  which 
explain  why  the  majority  of  partial  epilepsies  have  a 
somatomotor  or  tempororhinencephalic  symptoma- 
tology and  why  focal  frontal  or  occipital  epilepsies  are 
so  rare. 

Spontaneous  regional  hyperexcitability  and  hyper- 
excitability developing  around  and  remote  from  a 
cerebral  lesion  thus  play  a  fundamental  part  in  the 
development  of  seizures  of  partial  epilepsy.  It  is 
similar  to  the  part  played  by  general  neuronal  hyper- 


excitaljility  in  the  development  of  generalized  seizures, 
and  which  we  have  already  termed  a  'predisposing 
role'." 

The  precipitating  factor  is  also  the  same  in  the 
partial  as  in  generalized  epilepsies.  This  factor  is  a 
volley  of  afferent  stimuli  which,  although  without 
pathological  effect  under  normal  conditions,  can  pro- 
voke paroxysmal  hypersynchrony  when  local  hyper- 
excitability is  present.  The  precipitating  role  of 
afferent  stimuli  was  physiologically  demonstrated 
once  (and  perhaps  for  all)  by  the  remarkable  experi- 
ments of  Clementi  (28),  in  which  strychninization  of 
the  visual  cortex  is  in  no  way  detectable  until  \isual 
stimulation  is  applied,  whereupon  it  provokes  myo- 
clonic movements  of  the  eyelids  and  sometimes  even 
a  generalized  convulsive  seizure.  The  experiments  of 
Amantea  (8)  exemplify  the  same  principle,  showing 
that  strychninization  of  the  somatomotor  area,  is  in- 
sufficient to  produce  strychnine  clonus  yet  precipi- 
tates Jacksonian  or  even  generalized  seizures  when 
the  appropriate  reflexogenic  cutaneous  territory  is 
stimulated.  It  is  indeed  hardly  necessary  to  remind 
clinicians  of  the  numerous  cases  of  parietal,  temporal, 
amygdaloid  or  hippocampal  partial  epilepsy  precipi- 
tated by  an  unexpected  movement  (7),  a  noise  (13, 
74),  music  (33,  97),  rapid  ingestion  of  a  large  quantity 
of  water  (20)  or  an  emotion  (62,  i  78).'- 

In  most  cases,  however,  the  fact  that  afferent 
stimuli  precipitate  a  seizure  is  not  clinically  apparent 
because  local  hyperexcitability  increases  at  the  ap- 
proach of  an  attack  and  is  finally  so  marked  that  any 
volley  of  nervous  impulses  resulting  from  an  insignifi- 
cant stimulus  is  sufficient  to  fire  off  a  paro.xysm. 

PROPAGATION  AND  TERMINATION  OF  NEURONAL  DIS- 
CHARGE IN  PARTIAL  EPILEPSY.  We  have  already  seen 
that  a  localized  discharge  may  extend  locally  or  be 
propagated  concomitantly  or  successively  to  various 

"  An  epileptogenic  lesion  may  obviously  develop  in  a  patient 
with  a  predisposition  for  epilepsy  expressed  as  generalized 
neuronal  hypere.\citability,  either  constitutional  or  acquired. 
The  two  factors  are  then  added  together.  For  this  reason  a  ce- 
rebral lesion  will  frequently  provoke  seizures  of  partial  epilepsy 
in  one  subject  and  not  in  another.  For  the  same  reason,  Lennox 
found  a  degree  of  familial  predisposition  in  the  parents  of 
symptomatic  epileptics,  because  partial  epilepsy  dc\"elops 
particularly  in  those  who  are  already  so  predisposed. 

'-  Conversely  the  continuous  physiological  bombardment  of 
the  discharging  region  may  entail  its  desynchronization  and 
abort  a  seizure;  that  is  the  reason  why  certain  epileptics  abort 
their  somatomotor  or  psychomotor  fits  by  forcible  extension  of 
the  limb  in  which  the  jerks  first  appear  or  by  concentrating 
their  attention  fixedly  on  an  idea  or  a  perception. 


356 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


allied  structures  or  even  to  the  whole  brain.  Numerous 
authors  have  studied  propagation  of  epileptic  activity 
and  we  may  summarize  their  work  as  follows,  a)  Local 
propagation  of  the  discharge  proceeds  very  slowly 
like  a  '  drop  of  oil'  through  the  filjrillary  network  of 
grev  matter.  This  explains  the  Jacksonian  '  march' 
so  characteristic  of  seizures  provoked  by  the  discharge 
of  structures  somatotopically  arranged  (for  example 
the  jerks  which  extend  from  the  face  to  the  hand 
while  the  cortical  discharge  travels  from  one  repre- 
sentative part  of  the  cortex  to  the  next  adjacent 
part).  6)  Remote  propagation  takes  place  very 
rapidly  along  fibers  of  large  diameter.  This  explains 
the  almost  immediate  bringing  into  play  of  the  whole 
group  of  structures  of  a  corticosubcortical '  sector'  and 
the  stereotyped  symptomatology  during  one  or  subse- 
quent seizures  when  the  discharge  remains  localized 
to  such  a  sector.  On  the  contrary,  when  propagation 
takes  place  to  various '.sectors'  successively,  a  variety 
of  disturbances  appear  during  the  course  of  one  or 
subsequent  seizures. 

The  discharge  in  partial  epilepsy  is  propagated  by 
means  of  normally  functioning  nerve  fibers  and 
synapses  from  an  epileptogenic  center  which  is 
anatomically  altered  to  allied  centers  which  are 
anatomically  healthy.  This  implies  that  the  discharge 
originates  as  a  lesional  (or  more  likely  perilesional) 
phenomenon  but  that  its  propagation  is  an  exclusively 
functional  phenomenon.  Moruzzi  says:  "  It  is  ordinary 
nervous  activity  which  determines  that  allied  neurones 
pass  from  a  state  of  normal  activity  to  one  of  epileptic 
functioning." 

Although  involving  only  normal  functions,  this 
mode  of  propagation  is  nonetheless  pathological  since 
it  does  not  exist  in  the  normal  subject.  Indeed,  the 
following  two  conditions  are  necessary  for  its  produc- 
tion, a)  Hyperexcitaijility  of  the  neuronal  population 
allied  to  the  epileptic  center,  explaining  the  sensi- 
tivitv  \\  hich  it  acquires  under  epileptogenic  iiombard- 
ment.  VVe  have  already  shown  that  this  is  always  the 
case  in  generalized  epilepsy;  and  according  to  John- 
son &  Walker  (114):  "Not  only  the  primary  focus  is 
hypersensitive,  but  this  hypersensitivity  is  found  in 
the  other  cortical  and  subcortical  structures  with 
which  it  is  intimately  connected.  This  hypersensitivity 
manifests  itself  by  a  lowered  threshold  for  electrical 
and  chemical  stimulation  and  seems  to  result  from 
functional  disturbance  at  the  level  of  the  normal 
neurons,  as  a  result  of  the  influence  of  the  epilepto- 
genic focus." 

fe)  The  epileptic  discharge  cannot  be  propagated 
unless  the  bombardment  from  the  epileptogenic  cen- 


ter is  efficacious.  For  this,  it  requires  the  following 
properties:  the  bonii)ardment  discharges  must  be  of 
high  frequency  (1,000  cps);  these  discharges  must 
activate  a  sufficient  number  of  terminals  on  the  same 
cells  in  order  to  pro\oke  spatial  summation;  these  dis- 
charges must  be  rhythmically  spaced  so  as  to  use  the 
facilitation  of  supranormality  provoked  in  each  neuron 
by  the  previous  discharge  and  thus  to  produce  tem- 
poral summation;  and  the  bombardment  must  con- 
tinue long  enough  to  produce  a  progressive  effect. 

Although  hypcrexcitability  of  allied  centers  is  al- 
ways required,  all  the  conditions  necessary  to  make 
bombardment  effective  are  not  necessarily  present  at 
one  time.  Certain  ones,  indeed,  depend  on  the  func- 
tional or  anatomical  characteristics  of  the  bombarded 
or  bombarding  centers  and  of  the  pathways  which 
unite  them.  The  phenomena  of  spatial  summation, 
for  example,  depend  exclusively  on  the  number  of 
fibers  transmitting  the  bombardment  and  on  their 
mode  of  terminating  on  allied  neurons.  All  of  these 
conditions  vary  from  one  system  to  another  and  make 
certain  epileptic  propagations  easier  than  others. 

A  center  allied  to  an  epileptogenic  focus  reacts 
differently,  according  to  its  degree  of  excitability  and 
according  to  the  efficacy  of  bombardment,  a)  It 
may  remain  indifferent,  b)  Its  spontaneous  activity 
may  simply  i)e  increased,  r)  It  may  respond  stroke  for 
stroke  to  the  fjombarding  discharges  as  they  arrive; 
thus  true  evoked  potentials  are  produced  in  answer  to 
the  convulsive  waves  of  the  primary  focus,  with  a 
latency  corresponding  to  the  propagation  along  axons 
and  across  synapses.  Under  these  conditions  the  allied 
center  can  be  said  to  have  become  epileptic  because 
of  the  primary  focus,  d)  The  allied  center  may  become 
epileptic  on  its  own  account,  that  is,  it  may  dissociate 
itself  from  the  epileptogenic  focus  and  show  secondary 
autonomous  convulsive  activity.  This  may  persist 
after  the  end  of  the  primary  discharge  and  be  propa- 
gated to  allied  structures  as  a  tertiary  discharge  (so- 
called  '  erratic'  discharge). 

A  '  center'  secondarily  made  epileptic  by  bombard- 
ment from  a  primary  epileptogenic  focus  thus  modifies 
the  seizure  according  to  its  own  anatomical  and  func- 
tional characteristics.  One  of  two  things  usually  fol- 
lows; either  the  seizure  remains  partial  but  is  en- 
riched by  electroclinical  symptoms  consequent  upon 
the  new  discharge  and  this  discharge  may  lead  to 
another,  or  the  fit  becomes  generalized. 

In  the  first  case  a  seizure  may  begin  with  focal 
clinical  and  electroencephalographic  signs  and  pass 
through  a  series  of  equally  focal  episodes.  Many  psy- 
chomotor attacks  have  this  pattern,  notably  those  in 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


357 


which  an  occipital  EEG  discharge  accompanying  a 
visual  episode  follows  or  precedes  a  temporal  dis- 
charge with  aphasia  (60,  76). 

The  second  eventuality  explains  the  fact  that  any 
partial  seizure  may  become  generalized.  Generaliza- 
tion takes  place  more  readily  when  the  partial  seizure 
is  more  intense,  when  it  occupies  a  ree;ion  closely  con- 
nected to  the  centrencephalic  reticular  formation, 
and  when  the  patient  has  an  epileptic  predisposition 
or,  in  other  words,  generalized  neuronal  hyperexcita- 
bilitx'.  When  all  these  conditions  are  present,  the 
partial  seizure  becomes  generalized  almost  immedi- 
ately, and  the  localizing  signs  at  the  onset  may  pass 
unobserved.  One  must  therefore  always  question  the 
patient  and  eye  witnesses  closely  on  the  mode  of  onset 
of '  generalized'  seizures,  and  carry  out  an  EEG  exam- 
ination, even  when  the  diagnosis  .seems  indisputable, 
for  a  large  number  of  seizures  apparentK  generalized 
from  the  start  are  found  to  be  partial  epilepsy  second- 
arily generalized. 

The  evolution  of  partial  discharges  originating  on 
the  spot  (primary  discharges)  depends  upon  the  same 
factors  of  fatigue  as  in  generalized  discharges  and 
perhaps  also  on  the  same  phenomena  of  rhythmical 
inhibition.  For  this  reason  their  EEG  arrangement  is 
usually  the  same  as  in  generalized  discharges.  Rhyth- 
mic activity  is  first  sustained  in  the  saine  way  at  the 
initial  frequency  (indefatigability),  then  slowed  pro- 
gressively (growing  fatigability),  and  finalK'  inter- 
rupted by  episodes  of  silence  or  slow  waves  which 
grow  progressively  longer  (phase  of  exhaustion  or  in- 
hibition), until  at  the  end  there  is  silence  (phase  of 
postictal  extinction). 

On  the  other  hand,  partial  discharges  remote  from 
the  epileptogenic  lesion  develop  completely  differ- 
ently, in  a  way  which  defies  all  classification  because 
of  seizure  variability.  These  discharges  are  char- 
acterized by  slow  sinusoidal  or  notched  waves,  or  by 
polyphasic  spikes  with  an  initial  positive  phase,  and 
they  are  notable  for  their  long  duration  and  their  in- 
stability. At  one  moment  a  discharge  may  be  rhyth- 
mical and  of  large  amplitude  and  at  the  next  it  has 
lost  these  features.  The  discharge  may  be  slowed  or 
accelerated  indifferently  and  sometimes  even  pass 
through  two  or  three  successive  phases  of  speeding  up 
and  slowing  down.  Finally,  there  may  or  may  not  be 
postictal  extinction,  and  in  some  cases  the  record  be- 
comes normal  again  immediately  after  the  discharge 
has  ended. 

DISTINCTION  BETWEEN  TWO  GREAT  V.^RIETIES  OF  PAR- 
TIAL EPILEPSY  WITH  RESPECT  TO  CHARACTER  OF  THEIR 


DiscH.\RGES.  The  \arious  partial  epilepsies  have  often 
been  classified  according  to  the  structures  in  which  the 
seizure  develops,  or  at  least  in  which  it  originates. 
Such  a  conception  obviously  presupposes  that  the  dis- 
charge always  originates  in  the  same  place,  that  it  is 
always  propagated  along  the  same  pathways  and  that 
it  always  provokes  the  same  electroclinical  signs.  It 
also  presuppo.ses  that  the  first  symptom,  the  'signal- 
symptom'  or 'aura'  is  the  same  every  time,  points  in- 
fallibly to  the  site  of  the  lesion  and  guides  the  hand  of 
the  neurosurgeon. 

Such  rules,  however,  apply  only  to  a  small  minority 
of  the  partial  epilepsies,  namely  those  provoked  by  a 
very  limited  irritative  lesion  whose  discharge  involves 
a  closed  neuronal  system.  In  the  majority  of  cases, 
however,  these  rules  are  only  partially  applicable,  par- 
ticularly when  the  epileptogenic  lesion  is  extensive 
and  when  the  discharge  dev'elops  in  complex  neuronal 
systems  where  it  is  propagated  irregularly  and  differ- 
ently in  various  seizures  and  accordingly  provokes 
complex  and  variable  symptoms.  In  such  ca.ses,  the 
'signal-symptom'  is  clearly  less  valuable  (54,  76,  79), 
for  it  may  reveal  a  discharge  propagated  from  a 
clinically  silent  structure  and  it  may  \ary  from  one 
seizure  to  another  according  to  the  origin  and  propa- 
gation of  the  discharge. 

Two  varieties  of  partial  epilepsy  are  distinguished 
in  the  Marseille  school  (59),  according  to  propagation 
of  the  discharge  to  different  anatomical  systems. 

a)  In  the  first  variety,  the  causal  discharge  orig- 
inates in  a  structure  essentially,  if  not  exclusively, 
connected  to  one  single  other  structure.  Together 
they  constitute  a  limited  functional  system,  the  two 
'  poles'  (these  two  structures)  being  united  by  dense 
fibers.  In  this  system,  the  discharge  extends  from  one 
pole  to  the  other  but  always  stays  limited  within  the 
system,  for  although  other  fibers  unite  each  pole  to 
other  nervous  formations,  they  are  never  grouped 
sufficiently  densely  to  cause  effective  bombardment 
and  to  render  these  other  formations  epileptic.  The 
most  notable  examples  of  these '  bipolar'  systems  in  the 
brain  are  the  corticothalamic  sectors  connecting  the 
various  specific  areas  of  the  cortex  to  the  correspond- 
ing specific  thalamic  nuclei  (54). 

The  EEG  manifestations  consist  exclusively  of  dis- 
charges limited  to  the  sector  concerned  and,  in  con- 
sequence, are  recorded  from  a  very  localized  region 
of  the  scalp.  The  interseizure  discharges  consist  of 
sporadic  spikes  or  spikes-and-waves  which,  in  current 
EEG  usage,  reveal  an  'epileptogenic  focus';  the 
seizure  discharges  are  spikes  repeated   rhythmically 


358 


HANDBOOK   OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


and  slowing;  progressively,  which  constitute  a  '  partial 
seizure  discharge  expressed  focally'. 

It  is  evident  that  such  a  focus  or  discharge  does  not 
guarantee  that  the  epileptogenic  lesion  is  cortical,  for 
it  may  just  as  well  be  at  the  subcortical  pole  of  the 
system  and  nevertheless  be  expressed  in  the  cerebral 
cortex.  The  clinical  manifestations  of  the  seizures  de- 
pend upon  the  corticothalamic  sector  involved,  ap- 
pearing as  clonic  jerks  when  the  sector  of  precentral 
cortex  :^  ventrolateral  nucleus  is  involved;  dysesthesia 
for  the  sector  of  postcentral  cortex  ^  nucleus  ven- 
tralis  posterolateralis;  visual  phenomena  for  the 
striate  region  ^  lateral  geniculate;  and  auditory 
phenomena  for  the  superior  temporal  ;=i  medial 
geniculate. '^ 

The  discharges  are  not  necessarily  generalized 
throughout  the  whole  of  the  corticothalamic  sector. 
Some  part  only  may  be  involved,  for  example,  the 
Jacksonian  twitching  may  affect  only  the  face.  Simi- 
larly, several  adjacent  sectors  may  be  involved  con- 
comitantly or  successively;  for  example,  the  Jack- 
sonian jerking  may  accompany  or  be  followed  by 
dysesthesia  in  the  corresponding  part  of  the  body. 

i)  In  the  second  variety  of  partial  epilepsy,  the 
causal  discharge  originates  in  a  nervous  structure 
which  is  more  or  less  diffusely  connected  with  several 
other  cerebral  regions,  constituting  a  multiple  relay 
system.  These  systems  are  too  numerous  and  at 
present  too  ill-defined  to  be  described  fully.  In  addi- 
tion they  are  interconnected  and  a  given  cerebral 
structure  may  belong  to  several  of  them.  We  can  how- 
ever distinguish  two  great  rhinencephalic  systems: 
the  hippocampus  connected  on  the  one  hand  to  the 
limbic  lobe  and  on  the  other  hand  to  the  hypothala- 
mus and  tegmentum;  and  the  basal  rhinencephalic 
formations  (piriformoamygdaloid  and  olfactoseptal) 
connected  on  the  one  hand  to  the  orbitoinsulotele- 
temporal  cortex,  and  on  the  other  to  the  epithalamus, 
hypothalamus  and  tegmentum  mesencephali.  There 
is  also  the  most  rostral  part  of  the  reticular  formation 
of  the  brain  stem  which  projects  diffusely  from  the 
thalamus  on  to  the  whole  of  the  cerebral  cortex  and 
which  was  previously  discussed.  This  last  system  may 
be  activated  globally  by  way  of  the  reticular  afferents, 
as  in  generalized  epilepsy,  but  it  may  often  be 
brought  into  play  in  a  fragmentary  way  in  the  partial 
epilepsies.'* 

"  These  seizures  most  commonly  de\  elop  in  the  precentral 
cortex  ;=i  n.  ventrolateral  nucleus  sector,  not  because  it  more 
often  contains  the  epileptogenic  lesion  but  because  it  has  the  low- 
est convulsant  threshold. 

"  These  diffuse  systems  are  often  activated   in   the  partial 


The  clinical  manifestations  are  complex  because 
they  involve  simultaneously  or  successively  a  large 
number  of  structures  with  different  functions.  Sen- 
sory, mental  or  motor  symptoms  may  be  associated 
or  succeed  each  other  and  Gowers  (92)  has  described 
cases  in  which  a  dozen  visual,  auditory,  olfactory,  il- 
lusional,  hallucinatory  and  motor  symptoms  follow 
each  other  without  interruption. 

Vegetative  and  affective  manifestations  are  particu- 
larly important  since  the  discharges  usually  involve 
the  rhinencephalon  and  diencephalon.  These  fre- 
quently include  abnormal  epigastric,  abdominal  and 
precordial  sensations  with  reactional  gestures:  chew- 
ing, salivation,  deglutition,  and  imperious  needs  to 
eat,  urinate  or  defecate,  as  well  as  disorders  of  atten- 
tion, anxiety,  fear,  anger,  etc. 

There  usually  is  clouded  consciousness  and  the  ap- 
pearance of  more  or  less  complex  automatisms,  since 
these  discharges  disturb  the  functioning  of  a  large 
part  of  the  brain  and  usually  involve  some  of  the 
diffuse  cortical  projection  system  which  helps  to  regu- 
late cerebral  excitability  and  consciousness. 

The  electroencephalographic  manifestations  take 
the  form  of  seizure  discharges  which  may  be  classified 
as  follows  a)  Localized  discharges  appearing  as  spike 
rhythms  in  the  temporal  region  (with  anterior  tem- 
poral and  middle  temporal  electrodes)  or  in  the  occip- 
ital region  (with  occipital,  posterior  temporal  and 
posterior  parietal  electrodes),  according  to  whether 
the  discharge  develops  in  the  amygdalotemporal 
system  or  the  pulvinaro-occipitoparietotemporal 
sector. 

These  localized  seizure  discharges  are  usually  situ- 
ated on  the  same  side  as  the  interseizure  focus  and  its 
causal  lesions,  but  fairly  frequently  they  are  situated 
on  the  opposite  side  (82).  Such  independent  contra- 
lateral discharges  may  indicate  a  secondary  vascular 
extension  of  the  lesion  to  the  other  side  (6g)  but  may 
also  indicate  a  functional '  vmleashing'  of  these  homolo- 
gous contralateral  structures  which  have  acquired 
epileptogenic  potentialitv  through  being  bombarded 

(78). 

fe)  Diffuse  discharges,  constituted  by  a  rhythm  of 
waves  gradually  slowing  or  accelerating,  more  or  less 
generalized  over  one  or  both  hemispheres  but  often 


epilepsies  through  the  rhinencephalic  formations  for  two  reasons: 
a)  the  latter,  chiefly  the  hippocampus  and  amygdala,  aie 
frequently  the  seat  of  epileptogenic  lesions  (pararhinal  sclerosis 
in  the  so-called  temporal'  epilepsies);  and  6)  these  rhinenceph- 
alic formations  have  the  lowest  convulsant  threshold  of  all 
cerebral  structures  Csee  above). 


THE    PHYSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


359 


predominant  in  the  frontotemporal  region.  This  oc- 
curs when  the  discharges  develop  in  the  diffuse 
thalamocortical  system. 

f)  Complex  discharges,  in  which  localized  and 
diffuse  discharges  are  associated,  either  independently 
or  concomitantly,  and  if  the  latter,  either  in  or  out  of 
phase.  This  occurs  when  various  cortical-subcortical 
systems  are  brought  into  play  simultaneously  or  suc- 
cessively causing  "  erratic'  discharges. 

cf)  Localized  or  generalized  flattening  of  the  basic 
rhythm  occurs  when  the  structure  involved  in  the 
seizure  is  endowed  with  the  property  of  desyn- 
chronizing  the  cortical  electrical  activity. 

e)  There  may  be  no  EEG  manifestation  of  a  seizure 
at  all  when  the  discharge  invokes  subcortical  struc- 
tures with  very  poor  cortical  projection  or  when  it  is 
unable  to  cross  the  synapses  leading  to  the  cortex. 

The  interseizure  discharges  may  be  more  or  less 
diffuse  for  the  same  reason  as  the  seizure  discharges, 
but  they  are  most  often  localized  to  the  temporal 
region  (and  particularly  the  anterior  temporal)  or  one 
or  both  hemispheres.  This  particular  site  is  the  most 
common,  as  Gibbs  has  well  shown,  because  these  in- 
terictal  discharges  usually  originate  in  the  diseased 
cerebral  structures  with  the  lowest  convulsant  thresh- 
old, that  is  to  say,  the  tip  of  the  temporal  lobe  and  the 
basal  rhinencephalic  formations  (piriform  cortex, 
amygdala  and  hippocampus)  which  also  project  on 
to  the  teletemporal  region  (72,  121;  and  later  authors). 

Having  described  these  two  great  varieties  of  partial 
epilepsy  on  the  basis  of  pathological  physiology,  we 
shall  further  describe  them  in  terms  of  anatomy, 
etiology,  symptomatology  and  therapy. 

a)  The  localized  partial  epilepsies  not  only  show  a 
local  discharge  but  are  usually  caused  by  a  localized 
superficial  lesion,  either  atrophic  or  neoplastic.  The 
causes  are  not  numerous  and  include  open  head  in- 
juries with  well-defined  craniocerebral  wounds,  lo- 
calized infections,  chiefly  periarterial  or  perivenous, 
local  vascular  accidents  (malformations  or  throm- 
bcses)  and  small  cortical  or  paracortical  tumors.  These 
lesions  are  discrete  and,  because  they  interfere  with 
the  normal  functioning  of  only  a  small  amount  of 
cerebral  parenchyma,  the  patient's  mental  make-up 
is  usually  normal  between  seizures,  especially  from 
the  intellectual  point  of  view.  The  lesion  is  usually 
cortical  for  the  superficial  pole  of  the  corticothalamic 
sector  is  a  much  larger  area  and  is  more  vulnerable 
than  is  its  deep  pole.  Since  the  lesion  involves  the 
convexity  of  the  cortex  and  spares  the  rhinencephalon 
and  diencephalon,  there  is  usually  no  disturbance  of 


character  or  behavior  between  seizures.  On  the  other 
hand,  interictal  neurological  symptoms  are  relatively 
frequent  (mild  hemiplegia,  dysphasia  or  hemianop- 
sia) for  the  lesion  involves  a  corticothalamic  sector 
with  specific  functions.  Surgery  may  often  be  indicated 
when  medical  treatment  fails  in  this  type  of  partial 
epilepsy  because  of  the  precise  and  superficial  locali- 
zation of  the  lesion  and  because  of  its  small  size.  The 
operation  usually  is  easily  performed  and  yields  excel- 
lent results. 

6)  The  diffuse  partial  epilepsies  not  only  have  a 
diffuse  discharge  but  arise  from  diffuse  sclerosis,  pre- 
dominating in  the  inferomedial  aspect  of  the  hemi- 
sphere, the  'pararhinal'  region.  The  causes  are 
numerous  and  varied  ijut  may  be  divided  into  three 
main  groups,  depending  on  the  age  at  which  the 
lesion  is  acquired:  severe  and  prolonged  compression 
of  the  head  during  delivery  (156);  cerebral  edema  in 
infancy  or  early  childhood  which  accompanies  various 
disorders  clinically  misnamed  'encephalitis',  consist- 
ing of  status  epilepticus  with  coma  and  subsequent 
transient  hemiplegia  (57,  58);  and  closed  head  in- 
juries in  the  adult  (64).  The  principal  pathogenic 
mechanisms  in  these  three  conditions  are  wedging  of 
the  hippocampal  gyrus  and  the  blood  vessels  supply- 
ing it  into  the  tentorial  incisure  during  compression  of 
the  brain  at  birth,  or  during  intracranial  hypertension 
secondary  to  cerebral  edema  in  childhood,  and  in- 
jury of  the  orbitoinsulotemporal  region  by  the  sharp 
edge  of  the  lesser  wing  of  the  sphenoid  from  the  contre- 
coup  accompanying  closed  head  injuries.  These  two 
mechanisms  are  responsible  for  the  two  aspects  of 
pararhinal  sclerosis,  incisural  sclerosis  (156),  and 
vallecular  (perifalciform)  sclerosis  (53,  57)  which 
develops  in  relation  to  the  tentorial  incisure  and 
around  the  vallecula  sylvii  in  the  region  correspond- 
ing to  the  pararhinal  region. 

Because  the  lesions  responsible  for  psychomotor 
epilepsy  are  so  widespread  and  so  severe  and  are  lo- 
cated in  the  pararhinal  region,  these  patients  fre- 
quently show  interseizin-e  disturbances  of  intellect 
and  particularly  of  character  and  of  sexual,  alimen- 
tary and  social  behavior  (62).'-^ 

On  the  other  hand,  these  diffuse  and  deep  lesions 
do  not  involve  the  majority  of  the  corticothalamic 
sectors  and  the  important  projection  pathways  which 
explains  the  fact  that  interseizure  neurological  mani- 

'°  The  basal  part  of  the  rhinencephalon  acts  as  a  controlling 
and  regulating  system  of  complex  automatic  activities,  princi- 
pally those  adapted  to  the  seeking  of  the  opposite  sex  and 
to  the  pursuit,  intake  and  ingestion  of  food  (56).  See  the  chap- 
ters in  this  work  dealing  with  this  region. 


360 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


festations  are  rare.  Finally,  since  the  pararhinal  region 
is  so  deeply  situated,  surgery  is  difficult  and  only  ex- 
ceptionally indicated,  for  it  requires  systematic  an- 


terior temporal  lobectomy  (Penfield)  extended  to 
the  uncus,  amygdala  and  hippocampus,  or  selective 
amygdalohippocampectomy  (Niemeyer). 


REFERENCES 

I.   Adrian,  E.  D.  and  G.  Moruzzi.  J.  Physiol.  95:  ^7,  1939. 
J.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  G.  Moruzzi.  J.  Physiol.  97:  153,  1939. 

3.  .Ahlquist,  R.  p.  J.  Am.  Pharm.  A.  36:  414,  1947. 

4.  Ajmone-Marsan,    C.    and    M.    G.    F.    Fuortes.    Eleclro- 
encephalog.  &  Clin.  .Keurophysiot.  i  :  283,  1949. 

5.  AjMONE-M.ARSAN,  C.  AND  F.  M.^RO.SSERO.  Elertrocncepkalog. 
&  Clin.  Neurophjsiol.  2  :  133,  1 950. 

6.  Ajmone-Marsan,    C.    and  J.    Stoll,    Jr.    A.M. A.    Arch. 
Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  66 :  669,  1 95 1 . 

7.  Alajou.anine,  T.   and   H.   Gast.aut.   Rev.   neural.   93:   29, 

8.  Amantea,  G.  .Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  188:  287,  1921. 

9.  .Amantea,  G.  Arch.  sc.  biol.  12:  413,  1928. 

10.  Andy,  O.  and  K.  Akert.  Eleclroenctphalog.  &  Clin.  Neuro- 
physiol.  Suppl.  3:48,  1953. 

11.  Andy,  O.  and  K.  .Akert.  J.  .\furopalh.  &  Exper.  .Weunil. 

14:  198,  '955- 

12.  Arduini,  a.   and  G.  Moruzzi.   Eleclmencephalog.  &   Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  5:  235,  1953. 

13.  Arellano,  A.,  R.  Schwab  and  J.  Casby.  Eleclroencephalog. 
&  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  2:217,  1950. 

14.  AsUAD,  J.  L'Epilepsie  experimentale.  Contribution  a  l' etude  de 
la  Pathogenic  des  Convulsions.  Paris:  Masson,  1940. 

15.  B.aglioni,  S.  and  M.  M.agnini.  Arch,  fisiol.  6:  240,  1909. 

16.  Bechterew,  W.  J\eurol.  ^entralhl.   16:  199,  1897. 

17.  BicARD,  N,,  Y.  Gastaut  and  J.  Roger.  Epilepsia  4:  73, 

1955- 

18.  Binsw .ANGER,    O.    In    JVothnagel,    Handbuch    der    Steciellen 

Pathologic  iind  Therapie.  Vienna,  191 3.  [Cited  by  Muskens 

(149)]"^ 

19.  Bouche,  G.  Epilepsia  5:  18,  1956. 

20.  BouDOUREsquES,  J.  AND  H.  Gastaut.  Rev.  neurol.  89:  155, 

'953- 

21.  Bremer,  F.  Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  .Med.  46:  627,  1941. 

22.  Bremer,  F.  and  V.  Bonnet.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  45:  53,  1953. 

23.  BuBNOFF,  N.  AND  R.  Heidenhain.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  26: 
137,  1881-2. 

24.  Buser,  p.  and  D.  Albe-Fessard.  Compt.  rend.  Acad.  Sc.  236: 

1197.  1953- 

25.  Cadilhac,  J.  Thesis.  Montpellior,  France:  Universite  de 

Montpellier,  1955. 

26.  C.iiM^G,H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  16:221,  1953. 

27.  Chauchard,  p.,  H.  Mazoue  and  R.  Lecoq.  Presse  med.  16: 
220,  1945. 

28.  Clementi,  a.  Arch.  Jisiol.  27:  356,  1929. 

29.  Clementi,  A.  Arch,  fishol.  27:  388,  1929. 

30.  Cohn,  R.  A.M. a.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  71  :  699,  1954. 

31.  Creutzfeldt,   O.  Schweiz.  Arch.  .\'eurol.    u.   P.tychiat.   77: 
163,  1956. 

32.  Creutzfeldt,    O.    D.    and    R.    VV.    Mever-Mickeleit. 
Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  Suppl.  3:  43,  1953. 

33.  Critchlev,  M.  Brain  60:  13,  1937. 

34.  Cure,    C.   and   T.    Rasmussen.    Electroencephalog.  &    Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  2:  354,   1950. 

35.  Davies,  p.  W.  and  a.  Remond.  .-1.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis., 
Proc.  26:  205,  1946. 


36.  Davis,  L.  and  L.  J.  Pollock.  A.M. A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  20:  759,  1928. 

37.  De  Hass,  .'\.  M.  L.,  C.  Lombroso  and  J.  K.  Merlis. 
Electrocephalog.  &  Clin,  .\europhysiol .  5:  177,  1953. 

38.  Dempsev.  E.  VV.  and  R.  S.  Morison.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  135: 
293,  1942. 

39.  Dempsey,  E.  W.  and  R.  .S.  Morison.  .-tm.  J-  Physiol.  135: 
301,  1942. 

40.  Dempsey,  E.  VV.  and  R.  .S.  Morison.  .im.  J.  Physiol.  138: 
283,  1943. 

41.  Dusser  de  Barenne,  J.  G.,  C.  S.  Marshall,  \V.  S. 
McCuLLOCH  AND  L.   F.   NiMS.   .Am.  J.  Physiol.    124:  651, 

■938. 

42.  Dusser  de  Barenne,  J.  G.,  VV.  S.  McCulloch  and  L. 
NiMS.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  10:  277,  1937. 

43.  Erickson,  T.  C.  A..\i.A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  43:  429, 
1940. 

44.  Federov,  L.  N.  r^tschr.  ges.  exper.  Med.  72  :  72,  1930. 

45.  Federov,  L.  N.  ^tschr.  ges.  exper.  .Med.  72:  82,  1930. 

46.  Feindel,  W.  and  p.  Gloor.  Electroencephalog.  c?  Clin. 
Neurophysiol.  6:  389,   1954. 

47.  Ferrier,  D.    M'est  Riding  Lunatic  .isrlum  .Med.  Rep.  3:  30, 

'873- 

48.  Fessard,  a.  Conjerence  a  la  Semaine  .Neurophysiologique  de  la 
Salpetriere  consacree  aux  grands  prohlemes  de  I'Epilepsie.  Paris : 
Masson.  In  press. 

49.  Franck,  F.  and  a.  Pitres.  Arch,  physiol.  norm,  et  pathol. 
2:1,  1883. 

50.  French,  J.  D.,  B.  E.  Gernandt  and  R.  B.  Livingston. 
.A.M. A.  .Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  75:  260,  1956. 

51.  Fritsch,  G.  and  E.  Hitzig.  .Arch.  .Anal.  Physiol,  fi'iss.  .Med. 
37:  300,  1870. 

52.  Gastaut,  H.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  2 :  249, 

■950- 

53.  Gastaut,  H.  Les  Epilepsies.  Paris:  Flammarion,  1950. 

54.  Gastaut,  H.  Encyclopedic  .Medico-Chirurgicale,  Tome  .Neuro- 
logic, Fascicule  1 7008.  Paris :  Encyclopedie  Medico- 
Chirurgicale,  1 95 1 . 

55.  Gastaut,  H.  Rii'  neurol.  21  :  i,  1951. 

56.  Gastaut,  H.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  45:  1 17,  1953. 

57.  G.'iSTAUT,  H.  The  Epilepsies.  Electro-clinical  Correlations. 
Springfield :  Thomas,  1 954. 

58.  Gastaut,  H.  Comptes  rendus  du  Collogue  de  Marseille. 
Bruxelles:  Editions  Acta  Medica  Belgica,  1955. 

59.  Gastaut,  H.  Encyclopedia  M edico-Chirurgicale,  Tome 
.Neurologic,  Fascicule  37250.  Paris:  Encyclopedie  Medico- 
Chirurgicale,  1 955. 

60.  Gastaut,  H.  In:  Les  Grandes  Activites  du  Lobe  Occipital. 
Paris:  Masson,  1957. 

61.  Gastaut,  H.,  P.  Benoit,  M.  Vigouroux  and  A.  Roger. 
Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  6:  557:  1954. 

62.  Gast.aut,   H.    and   H.   Collomb.   .Ann.   med.-psychol.    112: 

657,  1954- 

63.  Gastaut,   H.   and   M.   Fischer-Williams.  Lancet  2 :  1018, 

iq.'i?- 


THE    PHVSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


361 


64.  Gastaut,  H.  and  Y.  Gastaut.  Rev.  iilo-neuro-Ophlal.  23 : 

257.  I95I- 

65.  Gastaut,  H.  and  J.  Hunter.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
Neuropkyiiol.  2:  263,  1 950. 

66.  Gastaut,  H.  and  J.  Hunter.  J.  p/iyswl.,  Paris  42:  592, 

1950- 

67.  Gastaut,  H.  and  H.  Lammers.  In:  Lis  Grandcs  Aclivites  du 
Rhinencephale.  Paris:  Masson.  In  press. 

68.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet,  M.  Badier  and  A.  Roger.  J. 
physiol.,  Paris  ^$:  737,  1951. 

69.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet,  E.  Beck,  J.  Cavanagh  and  A. 
Me^'ER.  In:  Comptes-Rendus  du  II  Culloqiif  biternalionale  sur 
r Epilepsie  Temporale.  Springfield :  Thomas,  In  press. 

70.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet  and  M.  Fischer-Williams. 
J.  Nerv.  &  Merit.  Dis.  127:  21,  1958. 

71.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet  and  H.  Regis.  Cump.  rend.  Soc.  de 
biol.  In  press. 

72.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet  and  .\.  Roger.  Rer.  neurot.  87: 
224,  1952. 

73.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Naquet,  R.  Vigouroux,  A.  Roger  and 
M.  Badier.  Rev.  neurol.  88:  310,  1953. 

74.  Gastaut,  H.  and  E.  Pirovano.  Arch,  psicol.  neurol.  e 
psichial.   10:  287,   1949. 

75.  Gastaut,  H.,  G.  Ricci  and  H.  Kugler.  Rev.  neurol.  89: 

I.  1953- 

76.  G.\staut,  H.,  G.  Ricci  and  H.  Kugler.  Rev.  neurol.  89: 

546,  1953- 

77.  Gastaut,  H.  and  A.  Roger.  Rev.  neurol.  84:  94,  1951. 

78.  Gastaut,  H.  and  A.  Roger.  In:  Les  Grandes  Aclivites  du 
Lobe  Temporal.  Paris:  Masson,  1955,  p.  83. 

79.  Gastaut,  H.  and  M.  Vigouroux.  Rev.  neurol.  In  press. 

80.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Vigouroux  and  M.  Badier.  Rev.  neurol. 

85:505.  1951- 

81.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Vigourou.x,  J.  Corriol  and  M.  Badier. 
J.  physiol..  Pans  43:  740,   1951. 

82.  Gastaut,  H.,  M.  Vigouroux  and  M.  Fischer-Williams. 
In:  Comptes-Rendus  du  II  Collogue  Internationale  sur  l' Epi- 
lepsie Temporale.  Springfield :  Thomas.  In  press. 

83.  Gastaut,  H.,  Vigouroux  and  R.  Naquet.  Rev.  neurol. 
87:607,  1952. 

84.  Gastaut,  H.,  R.  Vigouroux  .\nd  R.  Naquet.  Electro- 
encephalog. &  Clin.  .\'europhysiol.  5:  291,  1953. 

85.  Gauthier,  C  M.  Parm.\  and  A.  Zanchetti.  Electro- 
encephalog. &  Clin.  Neurophjsiol.  8;  237,  1956. 

86.  GiBBS,  F.  A.  AND  E.  L.  Gibbs.  A.M. A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 
Psychiat  35:  109,   1936. 

87.  Gibes,  F.  A.  and  E.  L.  Gibbs.  Alias  of  Electroencephalography 
(2nd  ed.).  Cambridge:  Addison-Wesley  Press,  1952. 

88.  Gibbs,  F.  A.,  E.  L.  Gibbs  and  W.  G.  Lennox.  Brain  60: 

377.  '937- 

89.  Glev,  p.,  M.  Lapipe,  J.  Rondepierre,  M.  Horande  and 
T.  TouCHARD.  J.  physiol.  palhol.  gen.  38:  132,  1941. 

90.  Gloor,   p.   Electroencephalog.  t2?  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.   7:  223, 

'955- 

91.  GooDDY,  W.  Brain  79:  167,  1956. 

92.  Gowers,  W.  R.  Epilepsy  and  other  Chronic  Convulsive  Diseases; 
their  Causes,  Symptoms  and  Treatment .  London :  Churchill, 
1 881. 

93.  Green,  J.  D.  and  R.  N.'VQuet.  IV  Congres  International 
d' Electroencephaloiiraphie  et  .Keurophysiologie.  Bruxelles:  Edi- 
tions Acta  Medica  Belgica,  1957,  p.  225. 


94- 

93- 
96. 

97- 


99- 
100. 

lOI. 


103. 
104. 

105. 


106 
107 
108 
109 


1 12 
"3 

114 
"5 

116. 
117. 


119. 
120. 
121. 
122. 

123. 
124. 
125- 

126. 
127. 
128. 


Green,  J.  D.  and  I.  Shim.'i.moto.  .'{..M.A.  .irch.  .Veurol  & 
Psychial.  70:  687,  1953. 

Gutierrez-Noriega,  C.  Rev.  neuro-psiquiat.  i  :  85,  1938. 
H.all,  M.  Synposis  of  the  Diastaltic  Nervous  System.  London: 
Mallett,  1850. 

Hamoir,  J.  L.  and  J.  TiTEC.A.  Rev.  neurol.  80:  635,  1948. 
Havne,  R.  a.,  L.  Belinson  and  F.  A.  Gibbs.  Electroenceph- 
alog. <S  Clin.  Neurophysiol .  i  :  437,  1949. 
HoEFER,  P.  F.  .\.  AND  J.  L.  PooL.  A. M.A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  50;  381,  1943. 
HoRSLEV,  V.  Lancet  2 :  1 2 1 1 ,  1 886. 

Hunter,  J.  and  D.  H.    Ingv.ar.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
.Xeurophystol.  7:  39,   1955. 

Hunter,   J.    and    H.   Jasper.    Electroencephalog.    &    Clin. 
Neurophysiol .  i  :  305,  1 949. 

Ingv.\r,  D.  H.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  33:  137,  1955. 
Jackson,  J.  H.  In:  Selected  Writings,  edited  by  J.  Taylor. 
London:  Hodder  and  Stoughton,  1931. 

J.\sper,  H.  In:  Epilepsy  and  Cerebral  Localization,  edited  by 
W.   Penfield  and  T.   L.   Erickson.   Springfield:   Thomas, 
1 941. 
J.^SPER,   H.   Electroencephalog  &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol .    i  :  405, 

1949- 

[.■VSPER,  H.  .\ND  C.  Aj.mone-Marsan.  a.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Ment. 
Dis.,  Proc.  30:493,  1952. 

Jasper,  H.,  C.  Ajmone-Marsan  and  J.   Stole.  .l.XI.A. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  67:  155,  1952. 
Jasper,  H.  and  J.  Droogleever-Fortuyn.  A.  Res.  Nerv. 
&  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  26:  272,  1947. 

J.\SPER,  H.  and  T.  C.  Erickson.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  5:  333, 
1 941. 

Jasper,  H.,  R.  Naquet  and  E.  King.  Electroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  7:  99,  1955. 
Johnson,  B.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  18:  189,  1955. 
Johnson,  H.  C,  K.  M.  Browne,  J.  W.  Markham  and 
E.  a.  Walker.  Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  73:  97,  1950. 
Johnson,  H.  C.  and  A.  E.  Walker.  Electroencephalog.  & 
Clm.  Neurophysiol.  4:  131,  1952. 

Johnson,  H.  C  ,  .\.  E.  Walker,  K.  M.  Browne  and  J. 
J.   \V.    Markham.    A. M.A.   Arch.   Neurol.  &   Psychiat.   67: 

473.  '952- 

Jung,  R.  Arch.  Psychiat.  183:  206,  1949. 
Jung,  R.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  Suppl.  4: 

57,  1953- 

Juno,   R.   and  J.    F.   Tonnies.   .irch.   Psychiat.    185:   701, 
1950. 

Kaada,  B.  R.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  24:  Suppl.  83,   1951. 
Kaada,  B.  R.  .Nord.  med.  47:  845,  1952. 
Kaada,  B.  R.  .inn.  Rev.  Physiol.  15:  39,  1953. 
Kaad.\,  B.  R.,  p.  .\ndersen  and  J.  Jansen.  .Neurology  4: 

48,  1954- 

Karplus,  I.  P.   W'len.  klin.   IVchnschr.  27:  645,  1914. 

King,  E.  J.  Pharmacol.  &  Exper.  Therap.  116:  404,  1956. 

KiRiK.'\E,  T.,  J.  Wada,  Y.  Naoe  and  O.  Furuya.  Folia 

Psychiat.  .Neurol.  Japonica  7:  181,  1953. 

Kleitman,  N.  and  R.  M.'VGNUs.  Arch,  ges,  Physiol.  205:  148, 

19^4- 

Kopeloff,  L.  M.,  S.  E.  Barrera  and  N.  Kopeloff. 

.im.  J.  Psychiat.  98:  881,  1942. 

Kopeloff,   L.    M.,  J.    G.    Chusid   and   N.    Kopeloff. 

Neurology  4:  218,  1954. 


362 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


129.  KoPELOFF,  L.  M.,  J.  G.  Chusid  and  N.  Kopeloff. 
A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  74:  523,  1955. 

130.  Lavitry,  L.  Thesis.  Toulouse,  France:  Universite  de 
Toulouse,  1947. 

131.  Lewandowsky,  M.  and  G.  Fischer.  Die  Funhtion  des 
^entralen  JS'ervensy stems.  Jena,  1907.  [Cited  by  Marchand 
and  .Ajuriaguerra  (134).] 

132.  MacLean,  p.  D.  and  J.  M.  R.  Delgado.  Electroencephalog. 
&  Clin.  Neurophysiol .  5:  91,  1953. 

133.  Magoun,  H.  W.  and  R.  Rhines.  J.  Neurophysiol.  9:  165, 
1946. 

134.  Marchand,  J.  L.  and  J.  de  .Ajuriaguerra.  Epilepsies. 
Paris :  Desclee  de  Brouwer,  1 948. 

135.  Marinesco,  G.,  O.  S.^ger  and  .\.  Kreindler.  Rev.  neural. 
>:  '3-;9.  '932- 

136.  Markham,  J.  W.,  |.  M.  Browne,  H.  C.  Johnson  and  A. 
E.  Walker.  A.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  .Metil.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  282, 
1952- 

137.  Marossero,  F.  and  M.  Garrone.  Eleclroencephatog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  4:  23O;  1952. 

138.  McCuLLOCH,  W.  S.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clm.  Neurophysiol. 
1:19,  1949. 

139.  McCuLLOCH,   W.    S.    AND  J.    G.    DuSSER   DE    BaRENNE.    .Am. 

J.  Physiol.  113:  97,  1935. 

140.  McCulloch,  W.  S.  and  H.  \V.  Garol.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  4: 
555-  1941- 

141.  Mettler,  F.  a.  and  C.  C.  Mettler.  J.  .Heurophysiol.  3: 
527.  '940- 

142.  Morin,  F.  and  J.  D.  Green.  Anal.  Rec.  115:  433,  1953. 

143.  MoRisON,  R.  .S.  and  E.  W.  Dempsey.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  135: 
281,  1942. 

144.  MoRUZzi,  G.  Arch.fisiol.  44:  log,  1945. 

145.  MoRUZZi,  G.  L'Epilepsie  Expenmentale.  Paris:  Hermann, 
1950- 

146.  MoRUZZi,  G.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol .  .Suppl. 
4:  221,  1953. 

147.  MoRuzzi,  G.  AND  H.  W.  Magoun.  Electroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  .Xeurophysiol .  i  :  455,  1949. 

148.  Murphy,  J.  P.  and  E.  Gellhorn.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  8:  341, 

"945- 

149.  MusKENS.  J.  J.  L'Epilepsie.  .-Xnvers:  De  Vos-Van  Kleef, 
1926. 

150.  Naquet,  R.  Thesis.  Marseille,  France:  Universite  d'Aix- 
Marseille,  1953. 

151.  Nauta,  W.J.  H.  AND  D.  G.  Whitlock.  In:  Brain  Mecha- 
nisms and  Consciousness,  edited  by  E.  D.  Adrian,  F.  Bremer 
and  H.  H.Jasper.  O.xford:  Blackwell,  1954. 

152.  Noell,  W.  K.  and  E.  B.  D0MBR0VV.SKL  Cerebral  Localisa- 
tion and  Classification  of  Convulsions  Produced  by  .Severe  Oxygen 
Lack.  Randolph  Field,  Te.xas:  School  of  .\viation  Medi- 
cine, 1947. 

153.  OLM.STED,  J.   M.  D.  AND  H.  D.  EoGAN.  .{m .  J.  Phjsiol.  66: 

437.  1923- 

154.  Papez,  J.  W.  A.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  Proc,  21:21,  1942. 
135.   Parma,  M.  and  A.  Zanchetti.  .Im.  J.  Physiol.  185:  614, 

'956. 

156.  Penfield,  \V.  .A.m. a.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Paychial.  60:  107, 
1948. 

157.  Pike,  F.  H.,  C.  A.  Elsberg,  W.  S.  McCulloch  and  M. 
N.  Chappell.  a.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  7:  203, 
1931- 

158.  PoGGio,  G.,  E.  Walker  and  O.  Andy.  .•1..1/..1.  Arch. 
Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  75:  350,  1956. 


159.  Pollock,  L.  J.  and  L.  Davis.  A.  Res.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc. 
T-  158,  1931- 

160.  Pope,  A.,  A.  Morris,  H.  Jasper,  K.  Eliot  and  W.  Pen- 
field.  .-1.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  26:  218,  1947. 

161.  Prevost,  J.  L.  Encephale  i:  165,  1907. 

162.  Prus,  J.  IVien.  Uin.  Wchnschr.  11:857,  1898. 

163.  Ralston,  B.  and  C.  Ajmone-Marsan.  Electoencephalog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  8:  559,  1956. 

164.  Ranson,  S.  W.,  S.  W.  Ranson,  Jr.  and  M.  Ranson. 
.■1..V/..4.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  46:  230,  1941. 

165.  Ranson,  S.  W.,  S.  W.  Ranson,  Jr.  and  M.  Ranson. 
A.M. A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  46:  402,  1941. 

166.  Riser,  M.,  J.  Gayral  and  J.  Pigassou.  Bull.  Acad.  Med. 
■29;  257,  1945. 

167.  Roger,  A.  Thesis.  Marseille,  France:  Universite  d'Aix- 
Marseille,  1954. 

168.  RosENBLUETH,  .A.  AND  W.  B.  Cannon.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  135: 
690,  1941-1942. 

169.  RuF,  H.  NervenarrJ  22:  437,  1951. 

170.  RusSEL,  J.  Cases  oj  Suspended  Cerebral  Function  Occurring 
among  the  Phenomena  following  Epileptic  Fits.  Medical  Times 
and  Gazette,  1882. 

171.  Samaja,  N.  Rev.  med.  Suisse  Rom.  34:  77;  173,  1904. 

172.  ScHL.\G,  J.  D.  A.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  8: 
421,  1956. 

173.  ScHOEN,  R.  Arch,  exper.  Path.  u.  Pharmakol.  113:  257,  1926. 

174.  Schroeder  VAN  DER  KoLK,  J.  L.  K.  On  the  Minute  Struc- 
ture and  Function  of  the  Spinal  Cord  and  .Medulla  Oblongata 
and  on  the  Proximate  Cause  and  Rational  Treatment  of  Epilepsy, 
translated  by  W.  D.  Moore.  London :  The  New  Sydenham 
Society,  1859. 

175.  Segundo,  J.  P.,  R.  Naquet  and  R.  .\rana.  A.  M.  A. 
Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  73:  515,  1955. 

176.  Shimamoto,  T.  and  M.  Verzeano.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  17: 
278,  1954. 

177.  Shimizu,  K.,  S.  Refsum  and  F.  Gibes.  Electroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  4:  141,  1952. 

178.  Sorel,  L.  Arch.  Belg.  .\led.  Soc.  Hyg.  Med.  du  Trav.  et  Med. 
Leg.  13:594,  1955. 

179.  Speranski,  a.  D.  .4  Basis  for  the  Theory  oj  Medicine.  Mos- 
cow: INR.A  Cooperative  Publishins;  Society,  1935. 

180.  Spiegel,  E.  A.  .4m.  J.  Psychiat.  87:  595,  1930-31. 

181.  Spiegel,  E.  .A.  and  T.  Falkiewicz.  Arb.  Neurol.  Inst.  IVien 
28:  67,  1925. 

182.  Spiegel,  E.  A.,  H.  T.  Wycis  and  V.  Reyes.  Electroencepha- 
log. &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  3:  473,  1951. 

183.  Starzl,  T.  E.  and  H.  W.   M.iiGOUN  J.  .Veurophysiol.    14: 

133.  i95>- 

184.  Starzl,  T.  E.,  W.  T.  Niemer,  M.  B.  Dell  and  P.  R. 
FoRGR..\VE.  J.  .Veuropalh.  &  Exper.  .Neurol.  12:  262,  1953. 

185.  Stefens,  R.  and  J.  Droogleever-Fortuyn.  Schweiz- 
Arch.  Neurol,  u.  Psychiat.  72:  299,  1953. 

186.  Stole,  J..  C.  Ajmone-Marsan  and  H.  Jasper.  J.  .\euro- 
physiol.  14:  305,  1 95 1. 

187.  Temkin,  O.  The  Falling  Sickness.  A  History  of  Epilepsy  from 
the  Greeks  to  the  Beginnings  of  Modern  Neurology.  Baltimore : 
Johns  Hopkins  Press,  1 945. 

188.  Thom.as,  L.  B.,  R.  p.  Schmidt  and  .\.  A.  Ward.  Electro- 
encephalog. &  Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  7:  478,  1955. 

189.  Turtschaninow,  p.  Arch.  Exper  Path.  u.  Pharmakol.  34: 
208,  1894. 


THE    PH'iSIOPATHOLOGY    OF    EPILEPTIC    SEIZURES 


363 


190.  VON     BaUMGARTEN,     k.,     A.     MOLLICA     AND     G.     MORUZZI. 

Boll.  Soc.  ital.  biol.  sper.  29:  1376,  1953. 

191.  VuLPiAN,  A.  Gaz-  kehd.  med.  el  chir.  2-2:  202,  1885. 

192.  Walker,   A.   E.   and   H.    C.  Johnson.   .1.   Res.   .\erv.  (S 
Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  27:  460,  1948. 

193.  Walter,  W.   G.,  V.  J.  Walter,  H.   Gastaut  and  Y. 
Gastaut.  Revue  neurol.  80:  613,  1948. 

194.  Ward,  A.  A.  J.  Neurophysiol.  10.  89,  1947. 

195.  Ward,   A.   A.   and   M.   D.   Wheatlev.  J.  Neuropath.  & 
Exper.  Neurol.  6:  292,  1947. 


196.  Whitlock,   D.    G.,   a.    Arduini   and   G.    Moruzzi.   J. 
Neurophysiol.  16:414,  1953. 

197.  Williams,  D.  Brain  76:  50,  1953. 

198.  Wortis,  S.  B.  and  D.  Klenke.  Am.  J.  Psychiat.  12:  1039, 

'933- 

199.  Zanchetti,  a.  and  J.  M.  Brookhart.  J.  .Veuropliysiol.  18: 
288,  1955. 

200.  Ziehen,  T.  Dissertation.  Berlin,  Germany;  University  of 
Berlin,  1885. 

201.  Ziehen,  T.  Arch.  Psychiat.  20:  584,  1889. 


CHAPTER     XV 


Sensory  mechanisms — introduction 


LORD  E.  D.  ADRIAN     |      Trinity  College, Cambridge,  England 


THE  ESSENTIAL  ELEMENTS  of  the  sense  oigaiis  are  the 
receptor  cells  which  respond  to  physical  and  chem- 
ical disturbance  and  transmit  information  about  it 
to  the  central  nervous  system.  Naturally  in  these  days 
they  are  fascinating  material  for  the  cell  physiologist. 
The  electron  microscope  gives  him  new  data  about 
their  structure,  and  there  are  new  biophysical  and 
biochemical  techniques  for  investigating  their  re- 
actions. If  all  goes  well,  our  understanding  of  the 
changes  which  take  place  in  the  receptors  will  soon 
have  reached  the  molecular  level. 

The  sense  organs  also  provide  ample  material  for 
the  electrophysiologist  who  deals  with  them  as  con- 
stituent elements  of  the  nervous  system.  The  technique 
of  recording  nervous  activity  has  reached  great  pre- 
cision and  the  flow  of  information  can  be  studied  in 
the  cell  units  and  pathways  of  the  central  nervous 
system  as  well  as  in  the  peripheral  nerves.  In  the  ani- 
mal kingdom  there  is  still  a  vast  range  of  receptor 
apparatus  awaiting  investigation  and  even  in  the 
vertebrate  there  is  still  a  good  deal  of  exploration  to 
be  done,  particularly  about  the  receptors  which  signal 
internal  rather  than  external  events. 

Another  line  of  research  leads  beyond  the  receptors 
and  their  afferent  connections,  for  the  physiology  of 
the  sense  organs  must  include  the  study  of  their  func- 
tion as  well  as  of  the  properties  which  make  them 
react  to  the  stimulus.  Some  of  them,  pain  receptors 
for  instance,  may  be  no  more  than  warning  devices 
which  signal  whenever  their  environment  sets  them 
in  action,  but  many  are  used  actively  to  explore  the 
environment  and  such  use  involves  movement  directed 
by  the  central  nervous  system.  We  look  with  our  eyes, 
feel  with  our  fingers  and  sniff  to  identify  a  smell. 
Aclivit}-  directed  by  the  central  nervous  s\stem  may 
also  be  needed  to  protect  the  sense  organ  when  the 


stimulus  is  too  strong.  We  may  have  to  constrict  our 
pupils  and  shade  our  eyes,  or  cover  our  ears  or  hold 
our  ijreath.  Since  the  receptors  will  give  most  informa- 
tion when  the  stimulus  falls  within  a  particular  range 
of  intensity,  we  have  to  study  the  different  adjust- 
ments \vhich  keep  it  within  that  range. 

The  analysis  of  this  kind  of  central  control  has  been 
carried  out  most  fully  for  the  receptors  which  signal 
muscular  contraction.  The  muscle  spindle  is  a  sense 
organ  excellently  adapted  for  investigations  of  this 
kind,  for  in  it  the  signaling  and  adjusting  mechanisms 
are  coupled  together  in  a  single  structure  and  its 
function  is  to  guide  the  relatively  simple  operations 
involved  in  posture  and  limb  movement.  Recent  in- 
formation on  the  efferent  innervation  of  the  spindle 
has  given  us  a  much  clearer  picture  in  which  it  ap- 
pears as  an  active  participant  in  the  feed-back  mech- 
anism which  ensures  smooth  movement  against  a 
continuous    postural    background. 

The  action  involved  in  adjusting  the  stimulus  to 
the  sense  organ  can  vary  greatly  in  .scale  and  com- 
plexity, from  a  simple  reflex  contraction  to  an  elab- 
orate sequence  of  skilled  movement,  as  when  the 
microscopist  places  the  slide  in  position,  focuses  first 
with  the  coarse  adjustment  and  then  with  the  fine 
and  makes  appropriate  use  of  his  ocular  muscles,  ex- 
ternal or  internal.  In  such  operations  the  adjustment 
is  carried  out  by  muscles  in  the  organ  or  elsewhere. 
But  in  addition  we  may  have  to  consider  a  more 
direct  central  adjustment  which  does  not  operate 
throuQjh  the  muscular  link  but  by  efferent  nerve  fibers 
leading  directly  to  the  receptors  or  to  some  part  of 
the  pathway  from  them  to  the  central  nervous  system. 
At  present  we  know  that  there  are  efferent  fibers  to 
the  retina  and  the  olfactory  bulb.  There  are  indica- 
tions of  a  control  of  this  kind  in  the  cochlea  also  and. 


365 


366 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOOV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


although  we  do  not  know  ilK-ir  hinction,  \vc  know  that 
there  are  other  nerve  fibers  which  reach  the  peripheral 
receptors  but  are  not  directly  connected  with  the 
receptor  elements. 

All  the  actions  which  focus  the  sense  organs  on  the 
stimulus  will  evoke  afferent  signals  of  their  own  to  be 
related  to  the  signals  from  the  organ  itself.  Thus  the 
full  report  which  comes  to  the  central  nervous  svstem 
will  be  far  more  complex  and  informative  than  any- 
thing which  could  be  furnished  by  any  sense  organ 
isolated  from  the  body  and  controlled  only  by  the 
electrophysiologist. 

Our  primary  concern,  to  be  sure,  is  with  the  recep- 
tors and  their  reaction  to  the  stimulus.  How  and  to 
what  purpose  their  reaction  can  be  influenced  by  the 
central  nervous  system  opens  up  a  different  chapter 
more  concerned  with  the  central  than  the  peripheral 
mechanism.  But  the  receptors  are  there  to  decide  the 
line  of  behavior  which  the  organism  should  follow; 
they  have  to  supply  all  the  relevant  information  as  to 
what  is  happening  from  moment  to  moment,  and  from 
this  the  central  nervous  svstem  selects  the  items  of 
particular  importance.  It  is  essential,  therefore,  to 
consider  the  sense  organs  not  onlv  as  groups  of  recep- 
tors e.xcited  by  particular  physical  or  chemical  events, 
but  as  organs  capable  of  presenting  a  detailed  report 
which  will  enable  the  event  to  be  compared  with 
others  of  the  saine  class  which  have  occurred  before. 
The  description  must  ije  as  full  as  possible,  yet  it  has 
all  to  be  conveyed  l:)y  trains  of  impulses  in  nerve  fibers. 
Though  we  can  record  the  impulses  there  are  still  a 
good  many  problems  to  l)e  settled  before  we  can  reach 
a  clear  understanding  of  how  the  full  description  of 
the  stimulus  is  handed  on  to  the  brain. 

The  eye,  for  instance,  can  inform  us  that  there  are 
patches  of  light  on  the  retina  of  particular  shape,  in- 
tensity and  color.  We  suppose  that  the  shape  is  sig- 
naled by  the  distribution  of  the  nerve  fibers  which 
consey  the  signals  and  the  intensitv  by  the  number  of 
impulses  arri\ing  at  a  particular  region  of  the  nervous 
system  within  a  given  time.  Thomas  Young  suggested 
in  1807  that  the  color  may  be  signaled  by  particular 
nervous  elements  sensitive  to  particular  regions  of 
the  spectrum,  but  in  spite  of  the  many  fresh  data 
which  recent  work  has  given  us,  we  have  still  to  reach 
agreement  as  to  the  way  in  which  the  information  of 
color  is  combined  with  that  of  intensity  and  area. 

Again  we  are  aware  that  the  olfactory  organ  enables 
us  to  distinguish  an  immense  variety  of  odors.  We 
know  that  the  temporal  and  spatial  pattern  of  ex- 
citation in  the  organ  mav  varv  with  the  smell  and 


that  some  of  the  receptors  vary  considerably  in  their 
sensitivitN  to  different  kinds  of  odor.  It  seems  probable 
that  these  different  sources  of  information  can  be  com- 
bined to  give  the  full  range  of  discrimination,  but 
it  is  not  yet  clear  how  the  combination  is  achieved. 

The  receptors  in  the  skin  and  in  the  tissues  beneath 
can  give  a  great  deal  of  information  about  the  nature 
of  the  object  in  contact  with  it  and  active  exploratory 
movements  help  us  to  judge  shape,  size,  hardness,  etc. 
But  even  a  light  contact  on  a  passive  surface  will 
produce  a  discharge  of  impulses  in  a  variety  of  afferent 
fibers  of  different  diameter  and  rate  of  conduction 
coming  from  receptor  organs  of  different  structure. 
Zottcrman's  studies  of  the  temperature  receptors 
have  shown  that  these  at  least  form  a  group  with  a 
characteristic  structure  and  behavior.  With  the 
receptors  for  touch,  pressure  and  pain,  however,  .we 
are  still  ignorant  of  the  role  of  different  types  of  axon 
and  ending  in  producing  sensation  which  can  vary  so 
much  in  c|uality  and  in  the  attention  and  action  which 
it  will  arouse. 

With  all  this  to  occupy  us  at  the  periphery  we  need 
not  be  in  too  great  a  hurry  to  follow  the  sensory  dis- 
charge into  the  central  nervous  system  where  it  will 
be  far  less  easy  to  analyze.  But  there  is  one  problem 
which  deserves  mention  at  the  present  time  because 
we  may  be  already  on  the  way  to  its  solution,  or  at  all 
events  to  its  investigation.  It  is  the  problem  of  access 
to  the  higher  levels  of  the  brain.  The  sense  organs 
provide  a  running  commentary  on  a  great  varietv 
of  environmental  circumstances,  but  the  organism 
has  to  select  the  particular  reports  which  have  an 
important  bearing  on  its  present  and  future  behavior. 
The  classical  method  of  investigating  the  sense  organs 
by  comparing  stimulus  and  sensation  can  throw  no 
light  on  this  selectixe  treatment,  for  the  subject  has 
to  fix  all  his  attention  on  the  one  stimulus.  He  must 
look  for  a  feeble  illinnination  or  a  slight  change  of 
color  or  listen  for  a  faint  click  or  a  just  detectable 
change  of  pitch.  When  he  lectures  to  a  class,  however, 
such  stimuli  may  ha\e  no  effect  at  all  on  his  sensory 
experiences  or  on  his  course  of  action.  Indeed  this 
method  of  research,  though  it  can  tell  us  the  effect  of 
a  particular  sense  organ  on  the  attentive  mind,  cannot 
be  expected  to  tell  us  how  the  other  sense  organs  can 
be  prevented  from  reaching  it. 

This  problem  of  attention  is  not  likely  to  be  settled 
finally  until  we  know  far  more  about  the  processes 
involved  in  habit  formation,  in  the  factors  which 
attach  importance  to  particular  stimuli  and  in  those 
which   balance   conflicting   claims   from   moment    to 


SENSORY    MECHANISMS INTRODUCTION 


367 


moment.  Clearly  we  attend  to  stimuli  which  are  un- 
expected or  are  intense  in  themselves  or  likely  to 
give  rise  to  a  chain  of  activity  by  reason  of  past 
association,  but  the  afiferent  nerxous  discharges  must 
be  studied  at  all  levels  before  we  can  say  where  and 
why  some  fail  and  some  reach  through  to  conscious- 
ness.  Fortunately   the   investigation   of  the   reticular 


formation  has  given  a  new  impetus  to  the  studv  of 
attention.  With  modern  techniques  the  afferent  signals 
can  be  traced  in  their  passage  through  the  intact 
brain  and  we  can  e.xpect  that  soon  there  will  be  fresh 
data  bearing  on  this  penultimate  problem  of  the 
sense  organs.  The  ultimate  problem  of  their  effect  on 
the  mind  is  scarcely  one  for  the  physiologist  to  settle. 


CHAPTER   XVI 


Nonphotic  receptors  in  lower  forms 


H  A  N  S  J  O  C  H  E  M    A  U  T  R  U  M     |     Department  oj  ^oology,  University  of  Miinchen,  Germany 


C:  H  A  P  T  E  R  CONTENTS 

Protozoa :  Differentiation  of  Protoplasmic  Irritability 
Coelenterates :  Cnidoblasts  as  Independent  Effectors 
Higher  Invertebrates:  Emergence  of  True  Receptors 
Anatomical  Peculiarities 
Comparison  of  ttie  Senses  of  the  Invertebrates  with  Those  of 

Vertebrates 
Reactions  of  Simple  Receptors 
Specific  Types  of  Receptors 
Chemoceptors 
Proprioceptors 
Thermoreceptors 
Mechanoreceptors :  tactile  sense 
Mechanoreceptors :  vibration  sense 
Mechanoreceptors :  hearing 
Statocysts 


protozoa:  differentiation  of 
protoplasmic  irritability 

PROTOZOA  REACT  TO  STIMULI:  heat,  cold,  chemical  and 
mechanical  irritation,  gravity,  and  light  influence 
their  behavior.  These  stimuli  therefore  affect  the  pro- 
tozoan cells.  However,  it  is  a  significant  morphologi- 
cal and  physiological  problem  whether  sensitivity  to 
these  stimuli  is  limited  to  certain  parts  of  the  proto- 
zoan cell,  or  whether  the  whole  organism  can  be 
stimulated.  Only  if  the  former  is  true  can  we  speak  of 
receptors. 

The  bodv  protoplasm  and  its  surface  is  not  much 
differentiated  in  the  simpler  protozoa,  such  as  the 
amebae.  There  is  therefore  no  reason  to  look  for 
localized  receptors.  It  appears,  however,  that  the 
protoplasm  of  the  ameba  is  not  irritable  under  certain 
physiological  conditions.  The  ameba  does  not  react  if 
a  narrow  light  beam  strikes  the  hyalin  tip  of  the  outer 
end  of  a  pseudopodium  (80,  82,  83).  If  the  light  beam 


strikes  the  endoplasm  of  a  p.seudopodium  which  is 
streaming  toward  the  tip  (and  is  in  the  sol  state),  the 
streaming  of  this  pseudopodium  is  stopped  and  new 
pseudopodia  are  formed  in  other  parts.  If  the  light 
beam  strikes  the  plasmasol  some  distance  from  the 
tip,  streaming  will  be  accelerated.  Experiments  with 
ciliates,  such  as  Paramecium,  al.so  showed  the  suscepti- 
bility of  the  whole  body  to  stimulation  (61,  71,  72). 
Thus,  separated  pieces  of  cut  Paramecium  respond  to 
chemical  stimuli,  e.g.  by  0.5  to  i.o  per  cent  NaCl  or 
0.05  to  0.0 1  per  cent  H-iSOj,  and  to  temperature 
stimuli  in  the  same  way  as  do  whole  animals.  There 
is  also  no  difference  between  cut  parts  and  whole 
animals  in  the  response  to  gravity.  This  fact  is  of 
special  interest  since  the  sensitivity  to  gravity  depends 
on  the  principle  of  the  statocyst  (68,  69):  hea\ier  sub- 
stances included  in  the  body  exert  a  pressure  on  the 
underlying  protoplasm.'  However,  there  are  no  fa- 
vored locations  in  the  body  of  Paramecium  sensitive  to 
this  pressure;  it  can  be  effective  in  every  part  and 
may  produce  orientation  in  relation  to  the  gravita- 
tional field. 

In  contrast  to  Paramecium,  only  the  anterior  part  of 
the  ciliate  Sptrostomum  ambiguum  (which  can  grow  to 
4.5  mm  in  length)  is  sensitive  to  thermal  and  chemical 
stimuli  according  to  the  view  of  Alverdes  (6)  and 
Blattner  (18).  Excised  posterior  parts  swim  into  dilute 
picric  acid  without  reaction  (18).  However,  very  di- 
lute picric  acid  attracts  Spirosiomum  and  is  less  toxic  to 
it  than  to  other  ciliates  such  as  Paramecium  and 
Stentor.  Therefore  the  findings  of  Blattner  cannot  be 


'  It  is  not  known  which  inclusion  bodies  serve  as  statoliths 
causing  excitation  by  the  pressure  they  exert  under  normal 
conditions.  Koehler  assumes  that  all  inclusion  bodies  may  func- 
tion as  statoliths.  They  have  to  be  only  heavier  than  the  cyto- 
plasm (as  for  example  the  nucleus,  the  content  of  vacuoles  and 
iron  particles  in  experiments). 


?)^9 


370 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


taken  necessarily  as  proof  that  the  posterior  part  of 
Spirostomum  is  not  irritable  to  chemicals.  This  form 
responds  to  other  chemical  and  mechanical  stimuli  in 
the  anterior  and  posterior  part. 

A  localized,  receptorlike  structure  has  so  far  been 
found  in  only  a  few  flagellates.  Many  phytoflagellates 
QEuglena  and  others)  possess  an  eyespot  which  con- 
tains the  pigment  astaxanthin  (135).  In  Euglena  there 
is  a  light  sensitive  plasma  spot  located  in  the  con- 
cavity of  this  eyespot.  The  eyespot  itself  probably  has 
a  screening  function  to  light  (81,  84).  There  are  other 
phytoflagellates  (e.g.  Chlamydomonai)  which  are  sensi- 
tive to  light  throughout  the  body,  even  though  they 
possess  an  eyespot,  as  shown  by  mutants  without  an 
eyespot  (52).  Therefore  the  question  as  to  whether 
the  eyespots  are  real  receptors  is  still  open. 

The  effector  system  of  the  protozoa  is  very  compli- 
cated. It  is  certain  that  in  no  case  do  protozoan  cells 
react  according  to  the  all-or-none  principle.  The  de- 
gree of  contraction  of  the  pseudopodia  of  rhizopods 
depends  on  the  intensity  of  stimulation;  the  stronger 
the  stimulation  the  farther  the  contraction  spreads 
along  the  excited  p.seudopodium;  it  will  spread  to 
other  parts  of  the  body  if  the  stimulus  is  sufficiently 
strong  (127).  The  contractile  stalk  of  vorticellae  can 
be  completely  or  partially  contracted  depending  on 
the  intensity  of  the  stimulation  (29,  64).  The  rhythm 
of  ciliary  movements  in  some  parts  of  the  body  of 
ciliates  can  be  modified  independenth  of  the  activity 
of  the  remaining  cilia  (65,  89). 


COELENTER.JiTES:   GNIDQBL.ASTS   .■XS 
INDEPENDENT  EFFECTORS 

The  cnidoblasts  are  cells  which  are  characteristic  of 
the  coelenterates  (Cnidaria,  polyps  and  medusae).  The 
intracellular  structures  of  the  cnidoblasts  are  nemato- 
cysts  which  consist  of  a  bubble-like  capsule.  The  free 
pole  is  a  long  hollow  thread  which  is  introverted  and 
coiled,  as  shown  in  figures  i  and  2.  The  opening  of 
the  capsule  is  usually  covered  by  a  cap.  On  discharge 
the  cap  bursts  open  and  the  thread  is  ejected  by 
eversion.  Different  types  of  nematocysts  are  found  in 
the  same  species.  Furthermore,  they  are  different  in 
different  species  (56,  63,  118,  138). 

So  far  as  analysis  with  ordinary  light  microscopes 
is  concerned,  nematocysts  are  the  most  complicated 
structures  formed  by  cells.  Some  cnidoblasts  carry  a 
fine  spine  or  a  cone  of  fused  cilia  on  the  free  end,  the 
cnidocil  shown  in  figure  i ;  others  lack  this  cnidocil. 
Cnidoblasts  which  are  not  yet  differentiated  (inter-- 


stitial  cells)  form  new  nematocysts  during  their  whole 
life;  the  cnidoblasts  migrate — sometimes  in  groups — 
into  or  between  ectodermal  cells  and  thus  form  bat- 
teries of  nematocysts. 

There  exists  a  large  number  of  morphologically 
different  nematocysts  (138),  but  only  a  few  types 
have  been  analyzed  physiologically.  They  show  char- 
acteristic differences  with  respect  to  irritability  and 
function. 

/)  The  desmonemes  (also  called  volvents)  and 
stenoteles  (also  called  penetrants)  are  used  for  catch- 
ing food.  They  explode  upon  simultaneous  stimulation 
by  chemical  and  mechanical  means  (such  as  aquatic 
food  organisms  and  meat);  the  cap  bursts  open  and 
the  thread  is  everted  within  3  to  5  msec.  The  thread 
of  the  stenoteles,  supported  by  spines  at  the  base, 
penetrates  the  body  of  the  food  organism  even  through 


FIG.  I.  Scheme  of  a  stenotele  nematocyst  and  its  discharge, 
a,  cnidoblast  (ez)  with  nucleus  (n),  nematocyst  (st),  and 
cnidocil  (en);  b,  stenotele  nematocyst  during  discharge;  c,  after 
discharge  showing  cap  (i),  spine  (sp),  and  ejected  thread  (th). 
The  cnidoblast  is  not  drawn  in  b  and  c.  Magnification,  555  X. 
[From  Kiihn  &  Schulze  (76).] 


FIG.  2.  Nematocysts  of  Hydra  (cnidoblasts  omitted),  a,  des- 
moneme  prior  to  discharge;  b,  same  after  discharge;  c  and  d, 
atrichous  isorhizas.  Magnification.  2200  X.  [From  Kiihn  & 
Schulze  (76).] 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS 


371 


a  well  developed  cuticle.  The  distal  end  of  the  thread 
of  the  stenoteles  is  open  and  by  this  means  the  poison 
stored  in  the  capsule  can  be  injected  through  the 
thread.  The  desmonemes  on  the  other  hand  have  a 
thread  which  is  closed  at  its  distal  end  and  it  winds 
only  around  the  spines  and  other  parts  of  the  food 
organisms. 

2)  The  atrichous  isorhizas  (also  called  small  glu- 
tinants)^erve  Hydra  by  attaching  the  tentacles  to  the 
ground  during  the  migration  of  the  polyps. 

5)  Finally  the  holotrichous  isorhizas  are  exclusively 
a  defense  mechanism.  They  e.xplode  only  upon  types 
of  stimulation  which  cause  no  feeding  reaction. 

The  discharge  of  the  nematocysts  occurs  only  upon 
direct  stimulation;  no  nervous  control  exists.  No  nerve 
fibers  can  be  found  which  lead  to  the  cells  containing 
nematocysts.  With  electrical  stimulation  only  the 
nematocysts  directly  stimulated  react  (88,  90).  Even 
repeated  rhythmic  stimulation  by  means  of  condensor 
discharges  never  causes  a  diflfusion  of  excitation  be- 
yond the  area  direc-tly  stimulated.  Thus  the  cnido- 
blasts  contain  irritable  structures  which  act  both  as 
receptors  and  effectors  and  are  independent  of  a 
nervous  system. 

Direct  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  nematocysts 
on  the  tentacles  of  Anemonia  or  of  the  penetrants  and 
volvents  of  Hydra  does  not  normally  lead  to  a  dis- 
charge even  though  the  cnidocils  present  are  diverted 
(for  example  by  epibiotic  protozoa  or  artificially  by 
chemically  very  clean,  rounded  glass  needles).  Neither 
is  discharge  obtained  by  chemical  stimulation  alone 
(such  as  extracts  of  meat  or  food  organisms,  proteins, 
amino  acids  or  sugar).  However  the  threshold  for 
direct  mechanical  stimulation  is  considerably  reduced 
by  chemical  stimuli  produced  by  the  food.  The  im- 
mediate releasing  stimulus  is  therefore  a  mechanical 
one  which  however  only  becomes  efTective  if  the 
threshold  is  lowered  in  advance  by  certain  substances 
present  in  the  food. 

The  nature  of  these  very  specific  chemical  sub- 
stances present  in  the  food  organisms  is  unknown. 
They  are  not  proteins  but  they  are  firmly  adsorbed  on 
the  proteins;  they  can,  however,  be  extracted  with 
ethanol  or  acetone  (88). 

The  atrichous  isorhizas  serve  to  attach  the  tentacles 
to  the  ground  during  the  inigration  of  the  polyps. 
They  never  respond  to  stimuli  arising  from  food  or- 
ganisms. Chemical  stimuli  such  as  extracts  of  food 
organisms  raise  the  threshold  for  this  type  of  nemato- 
cysts. The  duration  of  mechanical  stimulation  neces- 
sary to  bring  about  discharge  is  greater  for  atrichous 


isorhizas  than  for  stenoteles.  Food  inhibits  chemically 
the  discharge  of  the  atrichous  isorhizas  (36). 

In  summary,  it  may  be  concluded  that  nematocysts 
respond  to  a  mechanical  stimulus.  A  simultaneous 
chemical  stimulus,  by  raising  or  lowering  the  thresh- 
old, determines  which  kind  of  mechanical  stimulus 
will  explode  the  nematocysts.  The  change  of  the 
threshold  insures  that  the  reaction  will  be  appropriate. 

The  cnidoblast  is  therefore  a  unique  tissue  element. 
As  an  independent  effector  it  contains  sensory,  excitor 
and  effector  elements.  The  sensory  element  is  in  itself 
not  simple  and  functions  by  means  of  two  distinct 
sense  organs,  mechanical  and  chemical  in  nature.  The 
cnidae  may  in  fact  be  said  to  be  double  sense  organs 
as  well  as  effectors.  There  are  no  obvious  analogies 
to  this  in  the  tissues  of  higher  animals  (88). 


HIGHER    INVERTEBR.-kTES:    EMERGENCE 
OF    TRUE    RECEPTORS 

Anatomical  Peculiarities 

The  receptor  cells  of  the  invertebrates  are  always 
primary  sense  cells;  every  sense  cell  has  therefore  a 
centripetal  afferent  nerve  fiber.  This  is  also  the  case 
in  organs  which  in  vertebrates  have  secondary  re- 
ceptor cells,  for  example  the  static  and  auditory 
organs. 

In  the  simplest  case,  the  sense  cells  are  separate  and 
are  not  yet  united  into  an  organ.  Such  scattered  sense 
cells  are  found  in  all  classes  of  invertebrates.  They 
have  the  simplest  shape  in  hydroid-polyps  and  actin- 
iae; here  they  are  located  in  the  epithelium  and  have 
the  shape  of  epithelial  cells.  They  appear  in  the  ecto- 
derm as  well  as  in  the  entoderm  (fig.  3).  They  may 
be  absent  in  the  column  ectoderm  of  the  actinians, 
even  if  they  are  numerous  in  the  ectoderm  of  the  oral 
disc  (88).  Nevertheless,  the  column  is  sensitive  to 
mechanical  stimuli  from  the  en\ironment,  although 
4000  times  less  so  than  is  the  oral  disc  (91).  Such 
single  sense  cells  are  found  in  the  epithelium  of  lower 
and  higher  wonns  and  molluscs,  e.g.  Lumbricus  as 
shown  in  figure  4.  The  sense  cells  of  the  higher  in- 
vertebrates are  normally  located  subepithelially  and 
send  one  peripheral  fiber  into  the  epithelium.  These 
bipolar  sensory  neurons  are  illustrated  in  figure  5. 

These  single  sense  cells  may  ha\'e  an  auxiliary  ap- 
paratus; for  example,  the  hair-sensillae  of  the  arthro- 
pods. These  often,  but  not  always,  contain  only  one 
sense  cell  which  sends  a  peripheral  fiber  into  the  in- 
terior of  a   hair  which   was  formed   bv  the   cuticle 


(fig-  13)- 


37^  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 

Fig3 


FIG.  3.  Primary  sense  cells  from  the  tentacles  of  the  sea  anemone  Ceriantlms.  [From  Hanstrom  (50).] 
PIG.  4.  Primary  sense  cells  from  the  epithelium  of  the  earthworm  Lumbrkus.  [From  Hanstrom  (50).] 
FIG.  5.  Bipolar  sensory  neurons  from  the  skin  of  the  slug  Anon  ater.  [From  Plate  (9^).] 


FIG.  6.  Various  types  of  scolopophorous  sense  cells  (scolo- 
pidia)  from  the  chordotonal  organs  of  insects,  a  and  h,  am- 
phinematic  scolopidia;  f,  d,  e,  and/,  mononematic  scolopidia. 
a,  simple  chordotonal  organ;  b,  scolopidium  from  the  haltere 
of  a  muscid  (fly);  c  and  d,  scolopidia  from  the  tympanal  organ 
of  the  cicada  Cicadetta  coriaria;  e,  scolopidium  from  the  tympanal 
organ  and  /,  from  the  subgenual  organ  of  the  grasshopper 
Decticus.  af,  axial  fiber;  ch,  chitin  cuticle;  dc,  cap  cell;  ec,  en- 
veloping cell;  hy,  hypodermis;  li,  ligament;  sc,  sense  cell; 
si,  scolops  (apical  body  of  the  sense  cell);  tf,  terminal  strand  of 
the  sense  cell;  and  va,  vacuole.  [From  Weber  (137).] 


Some  single  sense  cells  show  a  further  anatomical 
differentiation.  They  have  several  short  ramified  fibers 
(dendrites)  which  lead  to  auxiliary  cells.  E.xamples  of 
these  are  the  stretch  receptors  at  the  joints  in  crusta- 
ceans (i,  5).  As  a  rule,  the  stretch  receptors  send  a 
fixed  number  of  dendrites  to  a  small  bundle  of  muscle 
fibers  (fig.  15). 

The  epithelial  sense  cells  may  be  located  in  groups 
and  thus  form  anything  from  primitive  to  highly 
specialized  sense  organs.  If  they  are  located  in  the 
epithelium,  they  may  often  carry  fine  hairs  on  their 
surface.  Specific  structures  are  often  found  in  the 
sense  cells;  the  most  complicated  of  such  intracellular 
structures  are  the  apical  bodies  (scolopidia)  in  the 
chordotonal  and  tympanal  organs  (35)  diagrammed  in 
figure  6  and  the  rhabdomeres  in  the  eyes  of  the  in- 
sects (39,  47). 

Some  sense  organs  of  invertebrates  contain,  side  by 
side,  sense  cells  which  are  morphologically  differen- 
tiated in  different  degrees.  An  example  is  the  sense 
cone  on  the  last  joint  of  the  antennae  of  the  Diplopoda; 
in  this  three  different  types  of  sense  cells  are  located 
closely  together  (fig.  7). 

The  sense  cells  of  an  organ  may  be  morphologically 
similar  but  differ  in  physiological  respects;  of  three 
sense  cells  which  are  found  in  the  chemosensory 
sensillae  on  the  labellum  of  dipterans  (flies),  only 
two  send  peripheral  fibers  into  the  chemoreceptive 
part  of  the  hair  (fig.    11).  These  two  cells  react  to 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS 


373 


FIG.  7.  Section  of  the  last  two  segments  of  the  antennae 
(7  and  8)  of  the  diplopod  Polydesmus  complnnatus.  ep,  epidermis; 
fg,  finger-like  organ  (function  unknown);  h,  sensory  hair; 
j,  skin  joint;  mu,  muscle;  n,  nerve;  p,  peg-Hke  sensilla;  sci,  sc;, 
and  SC3,  the  three  types  of  sense  cells;  tr,  trichogen  cell.  [From 
Plate  C92).] 


are  found  in  great  numbers  in  all  soft  skinned  inverte- 
brates. Up  to  the  present  they  have  not  been  found  in 
the  turbellarians  and  echinoderms — probably  for 
technical  reasons.  As  a  rule,  these  peripheral  fibers 
form  a  plexus  which  can  be  located  subepithelially  in 
the  connecti\e  tissue  or  subcuticularly  above  the  epi- 
thelium. Such  plexuses  were  first  described  in  the 
classical  works  by  Retzius  (106)  and  von  Apathy 
(129).  In  arthropods  such  neurons  with  free  terminals 
are  limited  to  the  soft  skin  of  the  joints  and  that  be- 
tween the  segments  (fig.  8).  However,  they  are  found 
also  in  the  epithelium  (hypodermis)  ol  the  mouth 
parts  (fig.  9).  The  cells  01  these  neurons  are  mostly 
located  at  some  distance  from  the  terminal  ends  of 
the  dendrites.  Sometimes,  as  in  the  above  mentioned 
stretch  receptors  of  the  crustaceans,  these  dendrites 
are  short. 

The  simple  type  of  receptors  is  common  in  inverte- 
brates. On  the  other  hand,  some  very  complicated 
sense  organs  are  found,  for  example  the  phonore- 
ceptors  and  eyes  of  the  insects,  the  eyes  of  the  octopus, 
etc.  Even  in  its  highest  form,  however,  the  complexity 
never  reaches  that  of  the  vertebrates. 


ch.o 


different  chemical  substances;  one  neuron  reacts  only 

to  sugar  (with  spikes  of  smaller  voltage),  the  other  one 

(with  greater  spikes)  to  salts,  acids  and  alcohols  (60). 

Neurons  with  free  nerve  endings  in  the  epithelium 


FIG.  8.  The  trochanter  joint  of  the  third  pair  of  extremities 
oi  an  Aeschna  larva,  bp,  bipolar  sensory  neuron;  ch.o.,  chordo- 
tonal  organ  with  bipolar  sensory  neurons;  co,  coxa;  fe,  femur; 
sc,  sensory  neuron  with  dendritic  terminals  at  the  joint;  sh, 
sensory  hairs;  tl,  trochantinus.  [Redrawn  after  Zawarzin  (144).] 


374 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  9.  Sensory  neurons  from  the  hypopharynx  of  the  termite 
Cattolermes  flavicollis.  The  dendrites  run  between  the  cells  of  the 
hypodermis.  [From  Richard  (107).] 


The  morphological  differentiations  in  single  organs 
are  surprisingly  versatile.  [The  special  morphology 
and  anatomy  of  the  sense  organs  of  the  invertebrates 
may  be  found  in  the  extensive  monographs  of  Plate 
(92)  and  Hanstrom  C50).  They  cannot  be  given  here.] 
Physiological  analysis  has  fallen  far  behind  morpho- 
logical description.  The  function  of  most  structures 
and  organs  is  not  known  and  only  in  very  few  cases 
has  been  established  by  experimentation.  The  situa- 
tion here  is  similar  to  that  concerning  the  skin  recep- 
tors of  the  vertebrates.  The  number  of  these  is  far 
larger  than  the  number  which  has  been  analyzed  or  at 
present  can  be  identified  physiologically. 

Comparison  oj  the  Senses  oj  the  Invertebrates  with 
Those  of  Vertebrates 

The  functions  of  the  receptors  of  the  invertebrates 
are  known  in  detail  only  in  a  few  cases.  There  are,  in 
addition  to  apparently  very  primitive  organs,  some 
which  match  or  in  some  ways  even  surpass  the  effec- 
tiveness of  those  of  the  vertebrates.  The  absolute 
thresholds  (from  a  physical  point  of  view)  are  suffi- 
ciently well  known  only  in  a  few  instances  to  make 
possible  comparison  with  the  vertebrates.  For  the 
insects,  accurate  and  comparable  figures  are  available 
only  for  vibratory  and  auditory  reception  (lo).  The 
subgenual  organs  of  the  insects,  shown  in  figure  16, 
which  are  most  sensitive  to  vibration,  e.g.  in  Peripla- 
neta  and  Tettigonia  (9),  respond  to  amplitudes  of  vibra- 
tion of  the  ground  of  4  X  io~'"  cm  (with  an  optimal 
frequency  of  about  1400  cycles  per  sec).  The  thresh- 
old for  the  perception  of  vibration  in  the  human  on 
the  other  hand  is  about  10^^  cm  (67).  The  ampli- 
tude of  movement  of  the  membrane  of  the  human 
tympanum  at  the  threshold  of  hearing  is  of  the  same 


order  as  the  \ibration  threshold  in  the  insects  (143). 
The  thresholds  of  the  most  sensitive  sense  organs  of 
the  invertebrates  are  as  follows:  the  auditory  receptors 
of  the  grasshopper,  Tettigonia,  4  X  io~'^  watt  (11); 
the  subgenualorgan  of  the  cockroach,  Periplaneta, 
6  X  io~"  watt  (ii).  For  comparison,  the  auditory 
receptors  of  man  require  8  X  10"'*  to  4  X  io~'^  watt 
(120).  For  the  sense  of  smell  one  inay  also  assume 
that  the  best  threshold  \-alues  of  the  insects  match  or 
surpass  those  of  the  vertebrates.  The  receptors  of  the 
invertebrates  in  some  cases  show  potentialities  which 
are  not  known  in  the  vertebrates,  such  as  perception 
of  the  direction  of  vibration  of  polarized  light,  sensi- 
tivity to  ultraviolet  and  to  the  moisture  content  of 
the  air  (cf.  p.  376)  and  perception  of  ultrasound. 

The  means  by  which  comparable  results  are  ob- 
tained are  different  in  many  cases.  Vertebrates  gener- 
ally hear  well  in  the  range  of  16  to  20,000  or  50,000 
cycles  per  sec.  Insects  hear  soimd  oscillations  in  the 
range  to  about  300  cycles  per  sec.  by  means  of  their 
hair  sensillae  (103,  104).  The  ears  of  the  insects  which 
are  furnished  with  a  membrane  (tympanal  organs)  are 
actually  too  small  to  be  stimulated  by  air  vibration 
below  a  frequency  of  1000  cycles  per  sec;  they  are 
most  sensitive  in  the  ultrasonic  range  (beyond  10  to 
20  kilocycles  per  sec),  according  to  VVever  &  Bray 
(139),  Antrum  (9)  and  Pumphrey  (102).  Tympanal 
organs  cannot  distinguish  between  pitches.  Howe\-er 
they  are  very  sensitive  to  modulations  of  the  ampli- 
tude of  ultrasonic  waves  (53,  105)  up  to  modulation 
frequencies  of  more  than  300  per  sec.  (Antrum,  H., 
unpublished  observations);  different  frequencies  of 
modulation  can  be  distinguished.  Amplitude  modula- 
tion plays  practically  no  part  in  the  auditory  recep- 
tion of  the  vertebrates,  but  is  however  of  decisive 
importance  in  the  hearing  and  recognition  of  species- 
specific  .sounds  of  the  insects  with  a  tympanal  organ. 

Analogous  differences  of  functional  nature  appear 
if  the  photoreception  of  the  insects  and  vertebrates  is 
compared  (12,  14);  the  small  spatial  resolving  power 
of  the  complex  eye  of  the  flying  insects  is  compensated 
by  a  high  temporal  resolving  power.  The  frequency 
of  fusion  of  these  eyes  is  as  high  as  250  to  300  flashes 
per  sec. 

Many  proprioreceptors  of  the  arthropods  are  bas- 
ically different  from  the  corresponding  systems  in 
vertebrates  in  both  anatomical  and  physiological 
respects. 

Reactions  of  Simple  Receptors 

The  simple  receptors  of  invertebrates  serve  as  im- 
portant models  for  the  analysis  of  the  function  of  single 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS  375 


Fig.  10 


FIG.  10.  Sensilla  basiconica  from  the  antenna  of  a  pupa  of  the  wasp  Vespa  vulgaris,  ch,  chitin 
cuticle;  hy,  hypodermis ;  nl,  neurilemma;  sc,  sense  cells;  tf,  terminal  strands;  to,  tormogen  cell; 
tr,  trichogen  cell.  [From  Weber  (137).] 

FIG.  1 1 .  Diagram  showing  the  histology  of  a  labellar  hair  and  associated  cells  in  Phorima.  The 
large  trichogen  and  tormogen  cells  are  at  the  left,  and  three  neurons  with  silver  stained  processes  at 
the  right.  The  chemosensory  area  is  confined  to  the  silver -stained  tip  of  the  hair.  The  neuron  in  the 
middle  of  the  group  of  three  does  not  have  any  visible  connection  with  the  chemosensory  area. 
[From  Hodgson  &  Roeder  (60).] 


receptor  cells.  The  lateral  eyes  of  Lunulus  (sOj  the 
stretch  receptor  cells  of  crustaceans  and  the  chemo- 
receptor  sensillae  of  the  flies  are  examples.  It  has  been 
possible  to  analyze  the  functions  of  single  receptor 
neurons  in  these  simple  organs. 


Specific  Types  of  Receptors 

CHEMOCEPTORS.  The  chemoceptors  of  the  inverte- 
brates have  been  identified  by  physiological  experi- 
ments in  only  a  few  cases:  in  Turbellaria,  in  which 
the  auricular  organs  on  the  side  of  the  head  have 
been  studied  by  Koehler  (70)  and  by  Mljller  (87);  in 
Limulus  by  Waterman  &  Travis  (136)  and  Barber  (16); 
in  Crustacea  by  Hodgson  (58);  in  insects  by  von  Frisch 
(132),  Wigglesworth  (141),  Frings  &  O'Neal  (45), 
Frings  &  Frings  (44),  Hodgson  (57),  Grabowski  & 
Dethier  (48)  and  Hodgson  &  Roeder  (60).  [This  field 
has  been  reviewed  by  Dethier  (31),  by  Hodgson  C58) 
and,  particularly  for  molluscs,  by  Copland  (28).]  In 


the  turbellarians  and  molluscs  these  sense  cells,  which 
are  located  in  the  epithelium,  carry  fine  hairs  covered 
with  mucus.  Three  types  are  found  in  the  insects: 
sensillae  placodeae,  pore  plates  described  by  von 
Frisch  (132);  sensillae  basiconicae,  peg-like  hair  deriv- 
atives shown  in  figure  10;  and  sensillae  trichodeae,  hair 
sensillae  drawn  in  figure  1 1 .  They  are  always  supplied 
by  more  than  one  neuron.  The  covering  cuticle  is 
very  thin  (less  than  i  /x)  and  only  partially  sclerotized. 
The  epicuticle  has  a  low  lipid  content  (108).  The  sur- 
face of  the  cuticle  is  always  dry.  These  receptors  are 
suited  for  quantitative  experimental  comparisons  of 
different  substances.  [This  topic  has  been  reviewed  by 
Dethier  (31).]  Therefore  they  are  important  for  the 
general  physiology  of  chemoreception. 

In  the  vertebrates  we  distinguish  between  the  sense 
of  olfaction  and  the  sense  of  gustation.  An  analogous 
distinction  can  be  made  in  the  insects  but  not  in  other 
groups.  Hodg.son  (57)  showed  that  the  distinction  be- 
tween olfaction  and  gustation  is  unimportant  at  least 
on  a  cellular  level,  if  it  is  based  on  the  physical  condi- 


376 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'lnSIOI.OGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tion  of  the  stimulus.  A  small  group  of  morphologically 
identical  receptors  on  the  antennae  and  palpi  of  the 
beetle  Laccophilus  respond  in  the  same  way  to  chemical 
stimuli  by  suijstances  whether  they  are  dissolved  in 
water  or  applied  as  gases. 

Man\-  authors  assume  that,  besides  the  senses  of 
olfaction  and  gustation,  there  is  in  insects  a  common 
chemical  sense  with  separate  receptors  [cf.  Dethier 
(30)].  The  adequate  stimuli  for  this  common  chemical 
sense  are  high  concentrations  of  many  substances 
which  evoke  defense  reactions  (e.g.  ammonia,  chlo- 
rine, essential  oils). 

There  is  no  proof,  and  it  is  even  improbable,  that 
most  animals  can  distinguish  as  many  smells  as  can 
the  human.  Many,  if  not  most,  animals  are  probably 
specialized  and  able  to  respond  only  to  one  or  a  few- 
smells  in  a  very  specific  way.  The  females  of  Bomhyx 
mori  show  no  response  in  electrophysiological  experi- 
ments to  female  sexual  i)ait  substances  to  which  the 
males  react  with  marked  sensitivity.  It  may  therefore 
be  assumed  that  the  receptors  are  highly  specific  with 
respect  to  this  particular  substance  (113).  On  the 
other  hand,  the  olfactory  sense  of  the  honey  bee  is 
strikingly  similar  to  that  of  the  human  (131),  even 
with  respect  to  the  aiiility  to  distinguish  between 
stereoisomers  (e.g.  amyl  acetate  and  methyl  hepte- 
none;/)-cresol  methylether  and  m-cresol  methylether). 

For  the  human  sense  of  taste,  four  modalities  are 
generally  assumed :  sweet,  sour,  salty  and  bitter.  At 
present  it  is  difficult  to  say  whether  the  invertebrates 
have  more  or  fewer  of  these  modalities.  The  chemo- 
ceptors  of  Limulus  are  relatively  insensitive  to  salty, 
sweet,  sour  and  bitter  solutions  in  electrophysiological 
experiments.  However,  they  react  violently  to  water 
extracts  of  marine  clams  (16).  From  about  30  sub- 
stances which  taste  sweet  to  man,  only  a  few  are 
attractive  for  insects  (for  example  saccharine  is  not 
effective).  In  this  respect  not  only  different  insects, 
but  also  different  organs  of  the  same  insect,  react 
differently.  Raffinose  attracts  almost  all  insects  but 
not,  however,  the  bees;  the  ant  Lasiiis  niger  shows  a 
positive  reaction  to  sorbitol  but  the  ant  Myrmica 
rubida  does  not  (130,  133).  The  water  beetle  Hydrous 
is  able  to  distinguish  between  sugar,  hydrochloric 
acid,  sodium  chloride  and  quinine  in  behavior  experi- 
ments (17).  Frings  assumes  that  the  distinction  be- 
tween the  different  modalities  (salty,  sour,  sweet, 
bitter)  generally  is  not  dependent  on  the  presence  of 
different  specific  receptors  for  these  substances  in 
insects.  Stimulation  ot  the  receptor  cells  with  the 
lowest  threshold  is  supposed  to  cause  the  sensation 
'sweet'  and  the  e.xcitation  of  all  receptors  of  one  group 


to  cause  the  sensation  'sour'.  The  other  modalities 
would  be  based  on  the  evokation  ol  receptor  acti\ity 
patterns  which  lie  between  these  extremes. 

Many  terrestrial  invertebrates  respond  to  another 
modality,  moisture;  this  topic  has  been  reviewed  by 
Dethier  &  Chad  wick  (33),  by  Roth  &  Willis  (no) 
and  by  Dethier  (30).  The  moisture  receptors  of  the 
arthropods,  as  far  as  they  can  be  identified,  are  in- 
distinguishable from  the  other  chemoceptors  in  mor- 
phological respects.  According  to  experiments  by 
Dethier  (32)  it  is  however  very  dubious  whether  clean 
water  has  a  specific  'taste'  for  the  contact  receptors  of 
the  insects,  since  only  two  neurons  are  present  in  the 
hair  sensillae  of  the  fly  Phormia.  The  hair  can  be 
adapted  alternatively  to  water  and  to  different  con- 
centrations of  sugar;  an  alternative  adaptation  to 
sugar,  sodium  chloride  or  alcohol  (which  react  upon 
the  other  neuron)  is  not  possible.  According  to  these 
findings  there  is  only  one  receptor  for  sugar  and 
water.  A  similar  phenomenon  was  found  in  the  verte- 
brates (145).  It  is  not  possible  however  to  generalize 
and  to  apply  these  results  to  all  hygroreceptors.  It  is 
quite  possii:)le  that  specific  hygroreceptors  exist,  for 
instance  in  the  human  louse  Pediculus  humarus  corporis 
(.4.). 

The  chemoceptors  of  insects  are  remarkably  sensi- 
tive to  temperature  changes  (43).  The  neuron  which 
mediates  sodium  chloride  detection  in  the  fly  Plwrmia 
reacts  to  a  temperature  increase  of  o.i°C  with  a 
measurable  increase  of  spike  frequency  according  to 
Hodgson  et  al.  (39). 

Important  progress  has  been  made  in  recent  years 
in  the  electrophysiological  analysis  of  the  chemocep- 
tion  of  Limulus  and  insects  (16,  19,  60,  112,  113,  122). 
Hodgson  &  Roeder  (60)  observed  spikes  of  single 
neurons  of  chemical  receptors  in  insects.  Schneider 
(112,  113)  found  grouped  spikes  and  slow  potentials 
in  the  antennae  of  Bombyx. 

The  theories  concerning  the  primary  events  in 
chemoceptor  stimulation  will  not  be  discussed  here 
but  mav  Ijc  found  in  the  relevant  chapters  of  this 
work. 

PROPRIOCEPTORS.  Proprioceptors  are  defined  by  Liss- 
mann  (78)  as  sense  organs  capable  of  continuous 
registration  of  deformations  (changes  in  length)  and 
stresses  (tensions  and  compressions)  in  the  body.  In 
the  invertebrates  they  are  known  and  have  been  ex- 
perimentally tested  only  in  the  arthropods.  The 
following  types  can  be  distinguished  morphologically. 
On  the  surface  of  the  body  are  located:  a)  the 
peripheral    endings    of  multipolar    neurons    without 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS 


377 


FIG.  12.  Schematic  drawings  of  the  structure  of  an  insect 
campaniform  sensilla  (left)  and  an  arachnid  iyriform  organ,  a 
slit  sensilla  (right).  The  arrows  show  the  probable  direction  of 
the  stimulus  exciting  the  sensilla.  These  diagrams  are  based  on 
drawings  of  the  base  plate  sensilla  on  the  haltere  of  Calliphora 
(Pfiugstaedt,  191 2)  and  of  the  Iyriform  organ  on  the  patella  of 
a  spider,  ch,  chitin  cuticle;  hy,  hypodermis;  sc,  sense  cells; 
sf,  surface  (Vogel,  1923).  [From  Pringle  (97).] 


particular  differentiation  of  the  cuticle,  the  peripheral 
branches  of  which  terminate  between  the  cells  of  the 
hypodermis,  e.g.  in  the  skin  over  the  joints  in  the  ap- 
pendages of  Limulus  (16,  98),  or  in  the  crustaceans 
(126);  6)  campaniform  sense  organs  of  the  insects 
(fig.  12)  and  slit  sense  organs  of  the  Arachnoideae 
(fig.  12)  in  which  bipolar  sense  cells  send  their 
peripheral  fibers  to  special  diff"erentiated  structures  of 
the  cuticle  at  the  membrane  of  the  joints  (66,  95,  97, 
128);  f)  hair  sensillae  (sensillae  trichodeae),  which 
consist  of  single  or  larger  groups  of  hairs,  at  the  basis 
of  which  enter  the  peripheral  ends  of  bipolar  sense 
cells  (fig.  1 3) — because  of  their  location  they  are  more 
or  less  affected  by  the  relatixe  positions  of  adjacent 


segments  of  the  appendages  (95)  or  of  the  body,  e.g. 
of  the  head  according  to  Mittelstaedt  (86),  as  shown 
in  figure  14. 

In  the  interior  of  the  body  are  located:  a)  muscle 
receptors  in  insects,  as  shown  by  Finlayson  &  Lowen- 
stein  (40)  and  Slifer  &  Finlayson  (121);  ti)  organs 
suspended  between  two  movable  segments  found  in 
Limulus  (16,  98)  and  Crustacea  (especially  the  stretch 
receptor  organs  shown  in  fig.  15)  (i,  2,  3,  4,  22,  23, 
37,  38,  42,  74,  75,  140).  To  this  latter  group  also 
belong  with  high  probability  many  chordotonal  or- 
gans which  are  found  in  the  body  of  insects  (fig.  18). 
This  topic  has  been  reviewed  b\'  Eggers  (35)  and  by 
Snodgrass  (123). 

Proprioceptors  in  the  wings  of  insects  (124)  and  in 
the  abdominal  part  of  Dytiscus  (62)  have  been  found 
by  physiological  experiments;  they  are  however  not 
yet  identified  anatoinically.  In  earthworms  Gray  et  al. 
(49)  found  sensory  discharges  upon  passive  stretching 
and  Prosser  (100),  during  active  movement. 

The  adequate  stimulus  for  the  multipolar  sense  cells 
on  the  skin  of  the  joints  of  Limulus,  for  the  campani- 
form organs  of  the  insects  and  for  the  slit  sense  organ 
of  the  arachnoids  is  tension  or  coinpression  of  the 
cuticle  covering  the  organ.  The  receptors  in  the 
muscles  of  the  insects  and  the  stretch  receptors  of 
Limulus  and  of  the  crustaceans  respond  to  increase  or 
decrease  of  the  tension.  All  these  organs  are — with 
the  exception  of  one  of  the  two  neurons  in  the  stretch 
receptors  of  the  crayfish — tonic  receptors  with  slow 
and  incomplete  adaptation.  The  same  holds  true  for 
the  hair  sensillae  on  the  joints  of  the  insects.  Phasic 
receptors  in  close  proximity  to  the  tonic  neurons  are 
often  found.  They  respond  to  an  adequate  stiinulus 


FIG.  13.  Schematic  diagrams  of  hair  sensillae,  that  on  the  left  from  the  cercus  of  the  cricket 
Liogryllus  campeslris  with  an  intraepithelial  sense  cell,  and  that  on  the  right  from  the  caterpillar 
Pieris  with  a  subepidermal  sense  cell,  ch,  chitin  cuticle;  ha,  chitinous  hair;  hy,  hypodermis;  nl, 
neurilemma;  sc,  sense  cell;  to,  tormogen  cell  (secreting  the  chitinous  joint  membrane).  [From 
Weber  (137).] 


378 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  14.  Diagram  showing  the  method  of  excitation  of  the 
inner  coxal  hair  plate  by  a  fold  of  the  intersegmental  membrane 
of  the  second  leg  of  the  cockroach  Periplaneta.  ex,  coxa,  hp, 
hairplate;  pi,  pleuron.  [From  Pringle  (95).] 


with  a  short  series  of  impulses  and  rapidly  adapt; 
these  are  in  Limiilus  the  big  cells  located  in  the  depth, 
and  in  arachnoids  organs  which  are  not  exactly  iden- 
tified anatomically  but  are  located  close  to  the  slit 
organs.  The  stretch  receptors  of  some  crustaceans  are 
composed  of  two  sense  organs  lying  close  to  each 
other;  one  reacts  tonically  and  the  other  one  phasi- 
cally.  Each  of  these  sense  organs  consists  of  a  single 
neuron  which  is  located  on  a  bundle  of  muscle  fibers 


in  Homarus  and  Cambarus.  In  other  crabs,  such  as 
Carcinus,  many  cells  are  combined  in  this  organ.  Some 
of  these  cells  react  tonically,  others  phasically.  The 
tonic  neurons  show  a  resting  discharge.  The  maximum 
of  this  resting  discharge  of  single  neurons  in  Limulus 
is  present  either  at  maximal  extension  or  at  maximal 
flection  of  the  joint.  The  minimum  of  the  resting  dis- 
charge of  the  whole  organ  corresponds  therefore  ap- 
proximately to  a  mean  position  of  the  membrane  of 
the  joint 

The  stretch  receptor  organs  between  the  abdominal 
segments  of  certain  crustaceans,  including  Homarus 
and  Astacus,  have  been  studied  carefully.  They  are  of 
interest  also  from  the  standpoint  of  general  physiology 
since  the  activity  of  single  sensory  neurons  could  be 
analyzed  in  them.  The  organs  consist  of  two  fine 
bundles  of  muscles,  RMi  and  RM2;  a  sensory  neuron, 
.SNi  and  SN^,  is  attached  to  each  of  these.  This  neuron 
sends  many  dendrites  to  the  muscle  fibers.  An  afferent 
axon  is  emitted  from  each  neuron  (fig.  15).  The  effer- 
ent innervation  consists  of:  a)  motor  fibers,  innervat- 


FIG.  15.  Schematic  drawing  of  the  stretch  receptors  between  the  segments  of  the  tail  of  the  crab 
Astaau  flmnatilis.  RMi,  muscle  bundle  of  the  tonic  receptors;  RM.>,  muscle  bundle  of  the  phasic 
receptors.  SNj  and  SN^,  sensory  neurons  of  the  tonic  and  phasic  receptors  respectively,  Si  and  So 
their  afferent  axons;  MO],  three  thin  motor  fibers  of  the  tonic  receptors;  MO;,  thick  motor  nerve 
fiber  of  the  phasic  receptors;  and  I,  an  inhibitory  axon.  (By  courtesy  of  D.  Burkhardt.) 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    I.\    LOWER    FORMS 


379 


ing  each  bundle  of  muscles  separately;  b')  a  thick 
accessory  fiber  which  is  ramified  into  two  branches  in 
the  vicinity  of  SN2  (fig.  15),  one  branch  innervating 
the  area  of  the  dendritic  terminals  of  SNi,  the  other 
branch  innervating  the  dendritic  terminals  of  SNj; 
c)  an  additional  thin  accessory  fiber  innervating  the 
terminals  of  SNi  and  SN2  in  Homarus  (this  is  absent  in 
Astacus);  and  (f)  a  number  of  thinner  fibers,  the  origin 
and  terminals  of  which  are  not  known  exactly  but 
mainly  innervating  RMj. 

The  details  may  differ  in  the  different  species  of 
the  crustaceans  and  may  be  found  in  the  discussions 
by  Alexandrowicz  (i,  2,  5)  and  Florey  &  Florey  (42). 

The  adequate  stimulus  for  these  receptors  is  stretch- 
ing of  the  muscle  bundles.  One  of  the  two  receptor 
cells  of  this  organ  (SNo,  the  slow  cell)  has  tonic  quali- 
ties. The  other  one  (SNj,  the  fast  cell)  has  phasic 
qualities.  The  same  is  valid  for  the  corresponding 
muscle  bundles  on  efferent  stimulation,  RMi  yielding 
a  fast  twitch,  RM2,  a  slow  response  (74).  The  tonic 
receptor  cell  has  a  very  low  threshold  to  the  adequate 
stimulus  (stretch)  and  yields  uninterrupted  discharges 
with  a  low  adaptation  rate.  The  phasic  cell  has  a  high 
threshold  and  adapts  very  rapidly  and  completely. 
Excitation  of  the  efferent  nerve  fibers  leads  to  a  con- 
traction of  the  muscle  bundles  and  in  this  way  causes 
afferent  responses  of  the  sensory  neurons  (74,  140). 
The  normal  excitation  originates  in  the  dendrites. 
These  become  depolarized  by  stretch  deformation 
producing  a  generator  potential  (37,  38).  The  gener- 
ator potentials  in  the  dendrites  spread  electrotonicalh' 
and  reduce  the  resting  potential  of  the  cell  soma.  Re- 
duction of  the  resting  potential  (70  to  80  mv  with 
relaxed  receptor)  by  8  to  12  mv  in  the  slow  cell  and 
by  17  to  22  mv  in  the  fast  cell  causes  propagated 
impulses.  The  neuron  therefore  works  according  to 
the  following  scheme:  stretch  deformation  of  dendrite 
terminals— ^generator  potential^electronic  spread  to- 
wards the  cell  .soma  (prepotential) — >dendrite-soma 
impulse— >axon  impulse  (37,  38). 

Excitation  of  the  inhibitory  fiber  (fig.  15)  acts  upon 
the  generator  mechanism  in  the  dendrites  and  stops 
the  discharge  of  the  sensory  neuron  within  a  few 
milliseconds,  even  upon  application  of  a  strong  stimu- 
lus (large  stretch).  This  effect  is  caused  by  the  follow- 
ing sequence  of  events.  The  impulses  in  the  inhibitory 
fiber  cause  a  postsynaptic  effect  in  the  dendrites  of  the 
sensory  neuron.  This  drives  the  potential  of  the  re- 
ceptor cell  towards  an  equilibrium  level.  The  inhibi- 
tory effect  can  therefore  be  a  postsynaptic  depolariza- 
tion or  a  hyperpolarization  depending  upon  the 
existing  state  of  the  receptor  cell.  Through  the  stretch 


stimulus  or  its  absence  this  may  Ije  pushed  to  either 
side  of  the  equilibrium  potential  (37,  38). 

The  stretch  receptors  in  the  abdominal  segments  of 
the  crayfish  are  physiologically  similar  to  the  muscle 
spindles  of  the  vertebrates.  Nevertheless  all  described 
proprioceptors  are  considerably  different  from  the 
analogous  organs  of  the  vertebrates;  they  indicate  not 
the  functional  condition  of  a  single  muscle,  but  of  a 
whole  body  segment.  These  proprioceptors  signalize 
the  relative  position  of  the  parts  of  an  appendage, 
e.g.  the  hair  sensillae  or  the  multipolar  cells  oi  Limulus, 
or  they  indicate  the  relative  position  of  different  seg- 
ments of  the  body,  e.g.  the  stretch  receptors  of  the 
crayfish  or  the  muscle  receptors  of  insects.  The  cam- 
paniform  and  lyriform  organs  and  the  slit  sense  organs 
are  located  in  such  a  way  that  they  can  register  the 
forces  which  arise  in  the  chitinous  shell  of  the  legs 
upon  contact  of  the  extremity  with  the  ground.  These 
organs  therefore  control  the  behavior  and  position  of 
the  animals  (78,  93,  94,  95,  98). 

Many  of  these  organs  are  not  designed  to  react  to  a 
single  specific  mode  of  stimuli.  The  proprioceptors  of 
Carcinus  respond  both  to  the  movement  of  the  ex- 
tremity and  to  vibration  (22).  The  proprioceptors  of 
Dytiscus  (which  are  not  localized  anatomically)  yield 
spikes  during  inspiration,  expiration  and  to  airborne 
sounds  of  about  100  cycles  per  sec.  (62).  The  stretch 
receptors  of  Cambarus  respond  strongly  to  temperature 
changes  with  a  frequencv  change  of  the  discharges 
(40-^ 

THERMORECEPTORS.  Lower  animals  may  lack  tempera- 
ture sensiti\'ity  completely.  The  sea  anemone  CalUactis 
is  very  sensitive  to  mechanical  and  chemical  stimuli; 
however,  a  glass  tube  which  touches  the  body  wall 
can  be  heated  so  much  that  it  causes  burning  of  the 
ectoderm  without  producing  any  reaction  (91).  On 
the  other  hand  most  animals  react  to  temperature.  As 
a  rule  the  parasites  of  warm  Islooded  animals  are 
especially  sensitive,  beintr  attracted  by  warm  objects. 
This  has  been  shown  for  the  leech  and  some  insects, 
e.g.  Rhodnius  by  Wigglesworth  &  Gillet  (142)  and 
Cimex  by  Sioli  (119). 

The  temperature  receptors  of  invertebrates  have 
never  been  anatomically  localized  with  precision.  It 
is  assumed  to  be  highly  probable  that  the  pointed 
hairs  on  the  antennae  of  insects  are  thermoreceptors. 
This  is  the  case  in  at  least  some  species,  including 
Rhodnius  (142)  and  Pyrrhocoris  (46).  At  the  base  of 
these  hair  sensillae  lie  six  sensory  neurons. 

The  mechano-  and  chemoreceptors  of  the  inverte- 
brates very  often  respond  to  temperature  by  changes 


38o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH'SSIOLGGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


in  the  impulse  frequencies  in  the  afferent  nerves  (27, 
41,  43,  59).  The  Qio  in  the  statocysts  of  the  lobster  is 
4.5  for  the  nonadapting  resting  activity  of  the  large 
spikes,  according  to  Cohen  et  al.  (27).  This  tempera- 
ture sensitivity  is  therefore  lower  than  that  of  the 
temperature  receptors  in  the  tongue  of  mammals, 
studied  by  Hensel  &  Zotterman  (55),  and  of  the 
ampullae  of  Lorenzi  in  rays,  investigated  by  Hensel 
(54).  On  the  other  hand  Bullock  found  marked  me- 
chanical irritability  of  the  thermoreceptors  on  the 
head  of  the  rattlesnake  (21).  The  frequency  of  the 
afferent  signals  from  many  receptors  of  cold  blooded 
animals  depends  not  only  on  the  adequate  stimulus 
but  also  on  the  temperature.  This  temperature  sensi- 
tivity of  the  sense  organs  of  invertebrates  raises  a 
physiological  problem  which  has  hardly  been  investi- 
gated [cf.  Bullock  (20)]. 

A  summary  of  the  literature  on  the  reactions  of 
lower  animals  toward  temperature  is  given  by  von 
Buddenbrock  (130). 

MECHANORECEPTORS:  TACTILE  SENSE.  The  receptois  for 
this  modality  are  well  known  only  in  a  few  cases. 
Sense  cells  of  soft  skinned  invertebrates  which  are  lo- 
cated in  the  epidermis  and  carry  one  or  more  long, 
hair-like  spines  are  termed  hypothetical  tactile  re- 
ceptors. However,  no  full  proof  has  been  given  in  any 


case  [cf.  MiilkT  (87)].  Passano  &  Pantin  (91)  adopt 
the  view  that  a  basal  network  of  the  sensory  cells  or 
the  nerve  net  or  the  circular  and  parietal  muscle 
sheets  can  be  considered  as  receptors  despite  the  fact 
that  many  primary  sensory  neurons  occur  in  the 
ectoderm  of  the  actinians.  The  tactile  receptors  of  the 
insects  are  definitely  known  to  involve  long,  movable 
hairs  with  joints  in  the  chitinous  skeleton  at  the  base 
of  which  one  or  many  peripheral  fibers  of  bipolar 
sensory  neurons  end  (fig.  13).  They  adapt  rapidly  if 
their  resting  position  is  changed.  The  adaptation  is 
slow  in  certain  spine-like  hairs  located  on  the  legs  of 
insects.  The  initial  frequency  of  the  impulses  in  the 
sensory  a.xon  depends  on  the  velocity  of  displacement, 
according  to  Pumphrey  (loi).  The  transducer  func- 
tion of  this  sensory  element  was  analyzed  by  Pringle 
&  Wilson  (99).  They  were  able  to  show  that  the  maxi- 
mum of  response  (recorded  by  the  frequency  of  im- 
pulses) precedes  in  phase  the  maximum  tension  of  the 
stimulus  upon  application  of  harmonic,  sinusoidally 
varying  mechanical  stimuli.  This  is  a  corollary  of  the 
adaptation  shown  by  the  sensory  response  to  a  tran- 
sient stimulus. 

MECH.JiNORECEPTORS:  VIBRATION  SENSE.  Specific,  highly 
sensitive  viisration  receptors  were  found  in  the  ex- 
tremities of  insects  by  Autrum  (8,  1 1),  by  Autrum  & 
Schneider  (15)  and  by  Schneider  (115).  These  are 
groups  of  sensory  cells  which  are  spread  in  a  sail-like 
fashion  in  the  Ijody  fluid  of  the  legs  (fig.  16).  They 
are  furnished  with  peculiarly  differentiated  bodies, 
such  as  apical  bodies,  or  scolopidia,  as  shown  in  fig.  6. 
Adequate  stimuli  are  provided  by  vibrations  of  the 
ground.  The  subgenual  organs  respond  preferentially 
to  vibrations  between  200  and  6000  cycles  per  sec, 
with  maximum  sensitivity  between  1000  and  2000 
cycles  per  sec.  (fig.   1 7).  The  amplitude  at  threshold 


is  about  4   X 


"  cm  at  1500  cycles  per  sec.  for 


Fio.  16.  Scheme  of  the  subgenual  organ  in  the  leg  of  the 
ant  Formica,  ac,  accessory  cells;  ch,  chitin  cuticle;  dc,  cap  cells; 
ec,  enveloping  cell;  hy,  hypodermis;  nc,  nerve;  nl,  nucleus  of  a 
neurilemma  cell;  sc,  sense  cells.  [From  Weber  (137).] 


Periplaneta;  consequently  they  are  smaller  than  atoinic 
dimensions.-  The  adequate  physical  stimulus  is  ac- 
celeration. These  organs  cannot  distinguish  between 
different  frequencies.  The  high  optimal  frequency  of 
the  vibration  receptors  of  many  insects  given  in  figure 
I  7  can  be  understood  if  their  small  size  is  considered. 
Such  high  frequencies  do  not  occur  under  natural 
conditions.  It  is  therefore  not  necessary  to  distinguish 
the  frequencies.  Pulses  and  pulse-like  vibrations  of 
the  ground  are  important  for  reactions  of  insects  in 
the  natural  environment.  These  pulses  possess  high 
frequency  components  and  during  the  initial   tran- 

-  The  amplitudes  of  movement  of  the  human  tympanum  are 
of  the  same  order  of  magnitude  at  the  threshold  of  hearing. 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS 


381 


100.000  E3^ 


10.000 


1000- 


100  - 


10- 


c 

2    1 


0.1 


0.01 


0.001 


\^v 

\ 

v^\,Vespa 
^    \     .Carabus 

\       Bombus   \ 

\ 

\       X^Pyrameis       ^ 

Liogryi 

'^^^_ 

Penplanela— A      / 

1       1 

V     - 

00  o 

00  o 

000  o 

•—  CM 


Cycles    per    Second 

FIG.  17.  Vibration  thresholds  at  different  frequencies  for  a 
few  characteristic  species  of  different  sensitivity.  [From  Autriim 
&  Schneider  (15)-] 


sients  stimulate  the  subgenual  organs  at  their  reso- 
nance frequency.  The  suddenness  of  the  movement  of 
the  ground  is  therefore  important  for  successful  stimu- 
lation. This  holds  true  also  for  the  perception  of 
vibrations  in  spiders,  according  to  Liesenfeld  (77);  the 
vibratory  stimuli  which  are  emitted  from  the  threads 
of  the  net  require  a  sudden  onset  at  full  intensity, 
since  spiders  do  not  react  to  slowly  increasing  ampli- 
tudes. The  type  of  the  tran.sients  is  the  decisive  stimu- 
lus. The  same  is  true  for  the  t\nipanal  organ  of  the 
insects. 

MECH.\NORECEPTORS:  HEARING.  Specific  sen.se  organs 
for  which  air  sound  waves  are  the  adequate  stimuli 
are  known  in  arthropods.  These  are  the  hair  sensillae, 
the  antennae  or  the  tympanal  organs  which  contain 
a  membrane,  the  tympanum. 

The  hair  sensillae  serve  as  receptors  for  air  vibra- 
tion of  low  frequency  in  spiders  and  many  insects. 
The  reactions  of  caterpillars  (85),  and  the  hairs  on  the 
anal  cerci  of  the  crickets  and  cockroaches,  including 
Periplaneta,  (104)  have  been  carefully  analyzed.  The 
afferent  nerve  fibers  of  the  hair  sensillae  respond  to 


low  frequencies  up  to  about  400  cycles  per  sec.  in 
synchrony  with  the  frequency  of  the  stimulus  and  in 
some  cases  also  at  double  the  frequency.  At  higher 
frequencies,  halving  or  quartering  of  the  frequencies 
may  appear. 

The  antennae  of  Aedes  aegypti  (109),  Anopheles  (125) 
and  flies  (24)  carry  many  hairs  which  are  not  inner- 
vated. They  are  mo\ed  by  air  vibration  and  transfer 
this  motion  to  the  antennae  and  to  the  Johnston's 
sense  organ.  This  is  located  between  the  second  and 
third  segment  of  the  antennae  and  consists  of  many 
sense  cells. 

The  adequate  stimulus  for  the  hair  sensillae  is  the 
amplitude  of  displacement  of  the  air  particles  not  the 
sound  pressure,  according  to  Antrum  (7,  10)  and 
Pumphrey  (102).  They  are  thus  displacement  re- 
ceptors. 

The  tympanal  organs  are  sense  organs  with  a 
tympanic  membrane.  Their  primary  neurons  have 
scolopidia  (cf.  figs.  6  and  18).  The  structure  of  these 
organs  has  been  reviewed  by  Eggers  (35),  and  their 
physiology  by  Pumphrey  (102),  Autrum  (8,  9,  13) 
and  Schaller  &  Timm  (iii),  as  well  as  in  the  book 
edited  by  Busnel  (25).  The  maximal  sensitivity  is  in 


FIG.  18.  a,  chordotonal  organ  between  abdominal  segments 
of  the  larva  of  Monohammus  conjusor  (after  Hess).  [From  Weber 
(■37)]  *.  auditory  organ  in  the  foreleg  of  the  grasshopper 
Decticus  (after  Schwabe).  ch,  chitin  cuticle';  co,  sense  cells  of 
chordotonal  organ;  hy,  hypodermis;  li,  ligament;  nv,  nerve; 
sgn,  nerve  of  subgenual  organ;  ta,  anterior  tympanum;  tc, 
tympanic  cavity;  tp,  posterior  tympanum;  tr,  trachea.  [From 
.\utrum  (10).] 


382 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


the  supersonic  region,  above  20,000  cycles  per  sec,  as 
shown  by  VVever  &  Bray  (139),  Pumphrey  (102), 
Autrum  (8,  9,  10),  and  Schaller  &  Timm  (i  1 1).  The 
upper  limit  of  hearing  in  some  species  is  at  1 75 
kilocycles  per  sec.  Electrophysiological  analysis  has 
shown  that  the  tympanal  organs  cannot  distinguish 
the  frequency  of  the  sound.  They  consequently  are 
not  able  to  analyze  sounds  but  react  very  sensitively 
to  modulations  of  amplitude  (53,  105).  In  the  natural 
environment  the  stimulus  is  a  sound  of  high  frequency 
which  is  amplitude-modulated.  Such  a  sound  is  im- 
portant for  their  behavior.  For  example,  the  songs  of 
crickets  consist  of  sound  with  certain  rhythms  of 
modulation.  The  impulses  in  the  tympanal  nerve  do 
not  follow  the  frequency  of  the  sound  since  these 
frequencies  are  too  high  but  follow  the  modulation 
frequency  to  about  300  per  sec,  as  shown  in  Tetti- 
gonia  by  Autrum  (unpublished  observations).  The 
ultrasound  therefore  serves  as  a  carrier  frequency. 

Acoustically  these  tympanal  organs  are  displace- 
ment receptors  as  are  the  hair  sensillae  (8,  10,  102). 
The  sensitivity  of  all  acoustic  displacement  receptors 
depends  on  the  direction  from  which  the  sound  comes. 
The  tympanal  organs  have  for  this  reason,  therefore, 
an  8-shaped  polar  diagram,  which  is  important  for 
the  localization  of  the  source  of  the  sound  (10,  13). 
In  the  same  way  sound  reception  by  means  of  hairs  is 
dependent  on  the  direction  (125). 

ST.^TOCYSTS.  The  statocyst  of  the  invertebrates  consists 
of  a  little  ectodermal  bag  invaginated  into  the  interior 
of  the  body.  It  is  filled  with  fluid,  has  hairs  in  certain 
parts  and  contains  one  or  more  statoliths  which  are 
little  stones  or  concretions  of  greater  specific  gravity 
than  the  fluid.  According  to  the  general  scheme  of 
function  the  statocysts  are  analogous  to  the  otolith- 
containing  organs  of  the  vertebrates.  Considerable 
differences  are  found  in  the  anatomical  details.  Thus, 
the  sensory  neurons  in  the  invertebrates  are  primary 
sense  cells  with  an  afferent  axon  and  there  are  addi- 
tionally nonliving  cuticular  hairs  but  no  equivalent  of 
the  hair  cells;  in  the  vertebrates,  by  contrast,  the  hair 
cells  are  secondary  sense  cells  and  lack  an  axon. 

The  variety  of  the  details  is  also  very  great  in  the 
invertebrates  [cf.  Hanstrom  (50)  and  Plate  (92)]. 
Statocysts  are  the  first  sense  organs  to  appear  in  the 
phylogeny  of  the  animal  kingdom,  being  present  in 
the  coelenterates. 

The  adequate  stimuli  are  gravity  (static  stimuli), 
acceleration  (dynamic  stimuli),  or  both.  Compensa- 
tory reactions  of  the  whole  body,  tonic  reactions  to 
certain  muscles,  or  both,  are  directed  from  the  stato- 


cysts. The  statoliths  are  important  for  static  reactions 

(73). 

The  statocysts  of  crustaceans  have  been  most  care- 
fully analyzed  both  by  physiological  behavior  experi- 
ments by  Schone  (117)  and  Dijkgraaf  (34),  and  by 
electrophysiological  studies  by  Cohen  et  al.  (27)  and 
Cohen  (26).  The  adequate  stimulus  for  the  nerve  end- 
ing is  a  bending  of  the  cuticular  hair,  not  pressure  or 
pull  of  the  statolith  upon  the  hairs.  Bending  of  the 
hairs  medially  causes  a  refle.x  rotational  movement 
about  the  long  axis  in  the  same  direction;  bending 
towards  the  outside  causes  a  rotation  of  the  animal  in 
the  opposite  direction.  The  sensory  epithelium  gives 
rise  to  a  tonic  impulse  train  which  is  independent 
of  stimulation  of  the  statolith.  The  stimulus  bending 
the  sense  hairs  produces  either  its  own  impulse  or  may 
modify  the  tonic  impulses.  Between  the  intensity  of 
the  stimulus  (the  bending)  and  the  reaction  (measured 
in  the  tonic  reactions  of  the  eye  stalk)  there  exists  a 
linear  relationship.  If  the  statoliths  are  removed  on 
one  side,  the  zero  position  will  move  to  this  side  since 
the  area  of  the  statocyst  which  is  adjacent  to  the  sense 
hairs  is  inclined  outward  and  therefore  the  statoliths 
in  their  normal  position  bend  the  hairs  outward. 
Compensatory  processes  counteract  these  changes  of 
the  zero  position.  The  position  receptors  are  hook- 
shaped  hairs  in  Carcinus  and  Maja,  while  the  receptors 
for  angular  displacements  are  very  thin  thread-like 
hairs,  300  y.  long  (34).  The  statocysts  of  Astacus  do  not 
work  antagonistically  to  each  other,  but  in  the  same 
lateral  position  each  produces  the  same  tendency 
towards  rotation.  The  impulses  from  both  sides  are 
simply  added  up  in  the  central  nervous  system. 

The  results  of  Schone  (i  i  7)  and  Dijkgraaf  (34)  are 
in  accordance  with  the  electrophysiological  findings 
of  Cohen  et  al.  Q2'f)  and  Cohen  (26)  in  that  the  stato- 
cysts react  to  rotatory  acceleration  around  the  axes 
[cf.  Dijkgraaf  (34)],  to  linear  acceleration  and  to 
static  position.  A  tonic  discharge  exists  which  remains 
even  after  removal  of  the  statoliths.  There  exist  rapidly 
adapting  phasic  elements  and  also  tonic  elements. 
The  excitation  of  the  latter  depends  upon  the  position 
and  they  adapt  only  slightly.  Cohen  found  four  types 
of  afferent  fibers,  each  reacting  differently.  The  type 
I  position  receptor  shows  a  nonadaptive  impulse  fre- 
quency which  depends  (within  a  certain  angle  to  the 
normal  position)  upon  the  angle  between  the  trans- 
verse axis  and  the  normal  position.  The  position  re- 
ceptor of  type  II  may  well  be  not  a  single  receptor 
but  may  result  from  the  coordination  of  several  re- 
ceptors. It  a)  maintains  a  characteristic  nonadapting 
impulse  frequency  for  each  constant  deviation  from 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWXR    FORMS 


383 


the  horizontal  axis  and,  in  addition,  i)  yields  adapting 
impulses  during  movements,  which  increase  in  fre- 
quency if  the  movement  is  carried  out  towards  the 
position  of  the  maximal  static  stimulation  and  decrease 
abruptly  during  movement  away  from  this  position. 
A  third  system  of  receptors  responds  to  angular  ac- 
celeration around  every  axis  of  the  body.  It  shows  a 
tonic  discharge  which  is  independent  of  the  position 
when  position  is  constant.  This  tonic  discharge  re- 
mains even  after  removal  of  the  statolith.  The  response 
consists  of  a  burst  at  the  onset  of  rostrum-down,  side- 
down,  or  contralateral  horizontal  rotation,  followed 
by  a  depression  at  the  termination  of  these  move- 
ments. The  opposite  movements  result  in  a  reversed 
response  sequence.  The  adaptation  of  the  permanent 
discharge  to  the  resting  value  is  completed  in  less  than 
I  sec.  This  shows  a  striking  similarity  between  the 
basic  principles  of  the  mode  of  function  of  the  crusta- 
cean statocyst  and  the  static  apparatus  of  the  verte- 


brates studied  by  Lowenstein  (79)  and  von  Hoist 
(134);  in  both  cases  the  sine  law  is  obeyed;  the  ade- 
quate stimulus  is  bending  of  the  hairs;  stimulus  and 
reaction  have  a  linear  relationship;  and  the  sensory 
epithelium  emits  a  tonic  impulse  stream  which  is 
modified  by  the  bending  of  the  hairs.  A  loss  of  the 
tonic  impulses  on  one  side  is  compensated.  This 
ability  is  important  as  the  statocysts  have  to  be  sup- 
plied with  statoliths  from  the  outside  after  each  molt, 
at  least  in  some  species.  In  this  procedure  it  is  not 
always  possible  to  obtain  statoliths  of  the  same  weight. 
Orientation  to  gravity  occurs  in  many  cases  without 
the  help  of  statocysts,  as  is  the  case  in  insects  accord- 
ing to  Mittelstaedt  (86),  Pringle  (96)  and  Schneider 
(114),  but  the  eyes  often  play  an  important  role  in 
this  adjustment  (116). 

Professor  Autrum's  chapter  was  translated  from  the  Ger- 
man by  Dr.  and  Mrs.  Otto  Scherbaum,  Department  of  Zo- 
ology, University  of  California  at  Los  Angeles. — Ed. 


.S.  J.   mar.    hiol.   A.    V.   K.    31  :   277, 
S.    Puhhl.   slaz.   tool.   Napoli   25:   94, 


REFERENCES 

1.  .'\lexandrovvicz,  J.  S.  Quart.  J.  Mkrosc.  Sc.  92:  163,  1951. 

2.  .\lexandrowicz,   J.    S.    Quart.   J.    Microsc.   Sc.    93:   315, 
■952- 

3.  Alexandrowicz,  J. 

'952- 

4.  Alexandrowicz,  J. 

'953- 

5.  Alexandrowicz,  J.   S.  J.  mar.   hiol.   A.   U.  K.   35:    129, 

1956- 

6.  .-Klverdes,   F.  AV«f  Bahnpn  in  der  Lehre  vom  Verhalten  der 
niederen  Organismrn.  Berlin:  Springer,  1922. 

7.  .'^utrum,  H.  ^tsckr.  vergleick.  Physiol.  23:  332,  1936. 

8.  .\utrum,  H.  ^Ischr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  28:  326,  1940. 

9.  Autrum,  H.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  28:  580,  1941. 

10.  .^UTRUM,  H.  .Uaturivissensckaften  30:  69,  1942. 

11.  .^UTRUM,  H.  Biol.  Z^ntrathl.  63;  209,  1943. 

12.  AuTRUM,  H.  Naturwissenschajten  39:  290,  1952. 

13.  AuTRUM,  H.  Ann.  Inst.  .Nat.  Rech.  .igron.  Ser.  C.  Ann.  Epiphyt. 

6:  338,  195,5- 

14.  .Autrum,  H.  Exper.  Cell.  Res.  In  press. 

15.  Autrum,  H.  a.nd  VV.  Schneider,  ^tschr.   vergleick.  Physiol. 
31:  77,  1948. 

16.  Barber,  S.  J.  Exper.  Zool.  131:  51,  1956. 

17.  Bauer,  L.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  26:  107,  1938. 

18.  Blattner,  H.  Arch.  Protistenk.  53:  253,  1926. 

19.  Boistel,  J.  and  E.  Coraboeuf.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol. 
147:  1 1 72,  1953. 

20.  Bullock,  T.  H.  Biol.  Rev.  30:  311,  1955. 

21.  Bullock,   T.   H.   and   R.   B.    Cowles.   .Science    115:   541, 

1952- 

22.  Burke,  \V.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  31  :  127,  1954. 

23.  Burkhardt,  D.  Ergebn.  Biol.  20:  27,  1958. 

24.  Burkhardt,   D.   and   G.   Schneider,  ^tschr.  Naturjorsch. 
12b:  139,  1957. 

25.  Busnel,  R.  G.  (editor),  .inn.  Inst.  .Nat.  Rech.  Agron.  Ser.  C. 
.■Inn.  Epiphyt.,  vol.  6,  1955. 


26.  Cohen,  M.  J.  J.  Physiol.  130:  9,  1955. 

27.  Cohen,   M.  J.,  Y.   Katsuki  and  T.   H.   Bullock.   Ex- 
perienliag:  434,  1953. 

28.  Copland,  M.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  25:  177,  1918. 

29.  Danisch,  F.  ^tschr.  allg.  Physiol.   19:  133,   1921. 

30.  Dethier,   V.   G.    In:  Iruect  Physiology,   edited   by   K.    D. 
Roeder.  New  York:  Wiley,  1953,  p.  544. 

31.  Dethier,  V.  G.  Ann.  New  Tork  Acad.  Sc.  58:  139,  1954. 

32.  Dethier,  V.  G.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  30:  348,  1955. 

33.  Dethier,  V.  G.  and  L.   E.  Chadvmck.  Physiol.  Rev.  28: 
220,  1948. 

34.  Dijkgr.'\af,  S.  Experientia  11  :  407,  1955. 

35.  Eggers,  F.  Die  stiJtJUhrenden  Sinnesorgane.  Zool.  Bausteine, 
Bd.2.  Berlin:  Borntraeger,  1928. 

36.  Ewer,  R.  F.  Proc.  .^ool.  Soc.  117:  365,  1947. 

37.  Evzaguirre,  C.  and  S.  Kuffler.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  87, 

1955- 

38.  Evzaguirre,   C.   .\nd  S.   Kuffler.  J.   Gen.   Physiol.   39: 

121,  1955- 

39.  Fernandez-Mor.'\n,  H.  .Nature,  London  177:  742,   1956. 

40.  FiNLAYSON,   L.    H.   AND   O.    Lovvenstein.   .Nature,   London 
176:  1031,  1955. 

41.  Florey,  E.  ^tschr.  Naturjorsch.  lib:  504,  1956. 

42.  Florey,  E.  and  E.  Florey.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  69,  1955. 

43.  Frings,  H.  AND  B.  L.  Cox.  Biol.  Bull.  107:  360,  1954. 

44.  Frings,   H.    and   M.    Frinos.    Am.    Midland  .\atural.    41: 
602,  1949. 

45.  Frings,  H.  a.nd  B.  R.  O'Neal.  J.  Exper.  ^ool-   ^o'i-  61, 
1946. 

46.  Gerhardt,   H.   Zool-  Jahrb.,   Abt.   allg.   ^ool-   Physiol.   63: 

558,  1953- 

47.  Goldsmith,  T.   H.   and  D.    E.    Philpott.  J.   Biophys.& 

Biochem.  Cytol.  3:  429,  1957. 

48.  Grabowski,  C.  T.  and  V.  G.  Dethier.  J.  Morphol.  94: 
I,  1954- 


384 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


49.  Gray,  J.,  H.  \V.  Lissmann  and  R.  |.  Pumphrev.  J.  Exper. 
Biol.  15:  408,  1938. 

50.  Hanstrom,  B.  VeTgleichetide  Anatoniie  des  .Vfrrcnsyslems  der 
Wirbellosen  Tiere.  Heidelberg:  Springer,   1928. 

51.  Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  E.  F.  MacNichol, 
Jr.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Quant.  Biol.  17:  125,  1952. 

52.  Hartshorne,  J.  N.  Mew  Phytologist  52:  292,  1953. 

53.  Haskell,  P.  T.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  33:  756,  1956. 

54.  Hensel,  H.  ^hchr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  37:  509,  1955. 

55.  Hensel,  H.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav. 
23:  291,  1951. 

56.  Hertwig,  O.  and  R.  Hertwig.  Jena.  ^Lichr.  .Kalurw.  13: 

474.  '879- 

57.  Hodgson,  E.  S.  Biol.  Bull.  105:  115,  1953. 

58.  Hodgson,  E.  S.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  30:  331,  1955. 

■",9.  Hodgson,  E.  S.,  J.  Y.  Lettvin  and  K.  D.  Roeder. 
Science  122:  417,  1955. 

60.  Hodgson,  E.  S.  and  K.  D.  Roeder.  J.  Cell  &  Comp. 
Physiol.  48 :  5 1 ,  1 956. 

61.  Horton,  F.  J.  Exper.  Biol.   12:  13,  1935. 

62.  Hughes,  G.  M.  Mature,  London  170;  531,  1952. 

63.  HvMAN,  L.  H.  T/ie  Inirrtebrates.  I.  Protozoa  through  Cteno- 
phora.  London:  McGraw-Hill,  1940,  p.  382. 

64.  Jennings,  H.  S.  Am.  .Natural.  33:  373,  1899. 

65.  Jennings,  H.  S.  Behavior  of  Lower  Organisms.  New  York: 
Columbia  Univ.,  1906. 

66.  Kaston,  B.J.  J.  .Morphol.  58;  189,  1935. 

67.  Keidel,  VV.-D.  Vibrationsreception.  Erlanger  Forschungen 
Reihe  B:  .\aturwissenschaften.  Erlangen:  Universitatsbund 
e.V.  1956,  p.  I. 

68.  Koehler,  O.  .ir(h.  Protistenk.  45:  i,  1922. 

69.  Koehler,  O.  Arch.  Protistenk.  70:  279,  1930. 

70.  Koehler,  O.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  16:  606,  1932. 

71.  Koehler,  O.  Verhandl.  deutsch.  ^ool.  Gesellsch.  36  Q.^ool. 
Anz.  Suppl.  7):  74,  1934. 

72.  Koehler,   O.    Verhandl.   deutsch.   ^ool.    Gesellsch.    41  :    132, 

■939- 

73.  Kreidl,  a.  Sitzber.  .ikad.  Wiss.  Wien,  .Math.-nalurw.  Kl. 
102:  149,  1893. 

74.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  .Veurophysiol.  17:  558,  1954. 

75.  Kuffler,  S.  W.  .-^nd  C.  Evzaguirre.   J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39: 

155.  1955- 

76.  KiJHN,  A.  and  p.  Schulze.  In :  Kiikcnthal  Leilfaden  fiir 
das  ^oologische  Praktikum  (i  ith  ed.),  edited  by  E.  Matthes. 
Stuttgart:  Fischer,  1944. 

77.  LiESENFELD,  F.  J.  ^tschr.  Vergleich.  Physiol.  38:  563,  1956. 

78.  Lissmann,  H.  W.  Symp.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  4:  34,  1950. 

79.  LoWENSTEiN,  O.  Symp.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  4:  60,  1950. 

80.  Mast,  S.  O.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  9:  265,  1910. 

81.  Mast,  S.  O.  J.  E.x:per.  ^ool.  22:  471,  191 7. 

82.  Mast,  S.  O.  Biol.  Bull.  46:  55,  1924. 

83.  Mast,  S.  O.  Physiol,  ^ool.  5:1,  1932. 

84.  Mast,  S.  O.  Biol.  Rev.  13:  186,  1938. 

85.  Minnich,  D.  E.  J.  E.xper.  ^ool.  72:  439,  1936. 

86.  Mittelstaedt,  H.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  32:  422,  1950. 

87.  MuLLER,  H.  G.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  23:  253,  1936. 

88.  Pantin,  C.  F.  a.  ,7.  Exper.  Biol.  19:  294,  1942. 

89.  Parducz,   B.   Acta  microhiol.   .Acad.   Sc.   Hungaricae   I  :   1 75, 

■954- 

90.  Parker,  G.  H.  and  M.  A.  Van  .Alstvne.  J.  Exper. 
Zool.  63:  329,  1932. 


9" 

92 

93 
94 
95 
96 
97- 
98. 
99- 

100. 

lOI. 

102. 
103. 

104. 

105. 

106. 

107. 
108. 
109. 
1 10. 


1 12. 

'■3- 
114. 

'■5- 
116. 
117. 
118. 
119. 

120. 


122. 
123. 

124. 

■25- 
126. 

127. 

128. 
129. 
130. 

13'- 
132. 

'33- 


Passano,   L.   M.   and   C.   F.    A.   Pantin.   Proc.   Roy.  Soc, 

London,  ser.  B  143:  226,  1955. 

Plate,  L.  Die  Smnesorgane  der  Tiere.  //.  Allgemeine  ^oologie 

und  Abslarnmungslehre.  Jena:  Fischer,  1924. 

Pringle,  J.  VV.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  15:  loi,  1938. 

Pringle,  J.  \V.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  15:  114,  193B. 

Pringle,  J.  VV.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  15:  467,  1938. 

Pringle,  J.  W.  S.  Phil.  Trans.  233:  347,  1948. 

Pringle,  J.  W.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  32:  270,  1955. 

Pringle,  J.  \V.  S.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  33:  658,  1956. 

Pringle,  J.  \V.  S.  and  V.  J.  Wilson.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  29: 

220,  1952. 

Prosser,  C.  L.  J.  Exper.  Biol.  12:  95,  1935. 

Pumphrev,  R.  J.  J.  Physiol.  87:  6P,  1936. 

Pumphrev,  R.  J.  Biol.  Rev.  15:  107,  1940. 

Pumphrev,    R.   J.    and    A.    F.    Rawdon-Smith.    .Nature, 

London  137:  990,  1936. 

Pumphrev,   R.    J.   and  A.  F.   Rawdon-Smith.   Proc.  Roy. 

Soc,  London,  ser.  B  121  :  18,  1936. 

Pumphrev,    R.   J.    and    A.    F.    Rawdon-Smith.    .Nature, 

London  1 43  :  806,  1 939. 

Retzius,    G.    Biologische    Untersuchungen    N.    F.    4.    Berlin: 

Friedlander,  1892,  p.  i. 

Richard,  G.  Ann.  Sc.  .Nat.  ^ool.  13:  397,  1951. 

Richards,  A.  G.  Biol.  Bull.  103:  201,  1952. 

RfjTH,  L.  M.  Am.  Midland  .Natural.  40:  265,   1948. 

Roth,   L.    M.    and   E.    R.   Willis.   J.   Exper.   ^nol.    116: 

527.  195'- 

Sch.\ller,  F.   and  C.  Timm.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  32: 

468,  1950. 

Schneider,  D.  ^tschr.  .Naturjorsch.  lib:  121,  1956. 

Schneider,  D.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  40:  8,  1957- 

Schneider,  G.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  35:  416,  1953. 

Schneider,  W.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  32:  287,  1950. 

Schone,  H.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  33:  63,  1951. 

Schone,  H.  ^Ischr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  36:  241,  1954. 

Schulze,  P.  Arch,  ^ell/orsch.  16:  383,  1922. 

SiOLi,    H.   .^ool.  Jahrh.,   Abt.   allg.   ^ool.    Physiol.    58:   284, 

1937- 

.Sivian,   L.  J.   AND  S.   D.   White.  J.   Acoust.  Soc.   Am.   4: 

288,  1933. 

Slifer,  E.  H.  AND  L.  H.  FiNL.-wsON.  Quait.  J.  Microsc  Sc. 

97:617,  1956. 

Smyth,  T.,  Jr.  and  C.  C.  Ro^s.  Biol.  Bull.  108:  66,  1955. 

Snodcrass,  R.  E.  Principles  oj  Insect  Morphology.  London : 

McGraw-Hill,  1935. 

SoTov.'\LT.>\,  O.  Ami.  Entomol.  Fenn.  20:  148,  1954. 

TiscHNER,  H.  Acustica  3:  336,  1953. 

ToNNER,  F.  .^ool.  Jahrh.,  Abt.  allg.  ^ool.  Physiol.  53:   loi, 

'933- 

Verworn,    M.    Psychophysiologische    Prolistenstiidien.    Jena: 

Fischer,  1889. 

Vogel,  H.  Jena,  ^Ischr.  .Naturw.  59:  171,  1923. 

VON  Apathy,  S.  Mitt.  zool.  Stat.  .Neapel  12:  495,  1897. 

VON  BuDDENBROCK,  W.    Vcrglcichende  Physiologic.   L  Sinnes- 

physiologie.  Basel:  Birkhauser,  1952. 

VON  Frisch,  K.  ^ool.  Jahrb.,  Abt.  allg.  ^ool.  Physiol.  37 : 

I,  1919. 

VON  Frisch,  K.  I^ool.  Jahrb.,  Abt.  allg.  Z^ol.  Physiol.  38: 

449,  1 92 1. 

VON  Frisch,  K.  .^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  21:  i,  1935. 


NONPHOTIC    RECEPTORS    IN    LOWER    FORMS 


385 


1 34.  VON  HoLST,  E.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  32 :  60,  1 950. 

135.  Wald,  G.  Harvey  Lectures.  Ser.  41  :  117,  1945-46. 

136.  Waterman,  T.   H.   and  D.  F.  Travis.  J.  Cell  &  Camp. 
Physiol.  41  :  261,  1953. 

137.  Weber,  H.  Lehrhuch  der  Enlomotogie.  Jena:  Fischer,   1933. 

138.  Weill,  R.  Trav.  Slat.  zool.  Wimereux  10  u.   ii:  i,  1934. 

1 39.  Wever,  E.  G.  and  C.  W.  Brav.  J.  Cell.  &  Cnmfi.  Physiol. 
4:  79.  >933- 


140.  WiERSMA,     C.     .\.     G.,     E.     FURSHPAN     AND     E.     FlOREV.     J. 

Exper.  Biol.  30:  136,  1953. 

141.  WiGGLESWORTH,  V.  B.  Parasitology  33:  67,   1941. 

142.  WiGGLESWORTH,  V.   B.   AND  J.   D.   GiLLET.  J.   Exper.   Biol. 

11:  120,  1934. 

143.  WiLSK.A,  A.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  72:  161,  1935. 

144.  Zawarzin,  a.  ^tschr.  wiss.  zool.   100:  272,   1912. 

145.  ZoTTERMAN,  Y.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  18:  181,  1949. 


CHAPTER    XVII 


Touch  and  kinesthesis 


VERNON   B. 


JERZY  E.  ROSE 
MOUNTCASTLE 


Departments  0/  Physiology  and  Psychiatry,  Johns  Hopkins 
University  School  oj  Medicine,  Baltimore,  Maryland 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Introduction 
Definitions 

Tactile  stimuli 
Kinesthetic  stimuli 
Electrophysiological  Methods  for  Study  of  Somatic  Afferent 
Systems 
Methods  of  stimulation 
Excitability  states  of  central  neurons 
Methods  of  recording 
Current  Theories  of  Cutaneous  Sensations 
Classic  Concept 
Pattern  Theory 
Concept  of  Head 
Some     Properties    of    Peripheral     Somatic    Afferent    System 
Receptors 

Specificity  of  receptors 
Types  of  discharges 
Receptor  potential 
Peripheral  Cutaneous  Nerve  Fibers 
Impulses  in  peripheral  nerve  fibers 

Impulses  evoked  in  fibers  of  different  size  by  tactile  stimuli 
Relation    of  cutaneous    stimuli    to    activity    in    fibers    of 

different  size 
Relation  of  elevations  of  electroneurogram  to  modalities  of 

sensation 
Summary 
Central  Tactile  and  Kinesthetic  Systems 
General  Remarks 

Classification  of  Central  Tactile  and  Kinesthetic  Systems 
Medial  Lemniscal  System 
Anatomical  Definition 
Physiological  Properties 
Projection  Patterns  in  Medial  Lemniscal  System 
Patterns  in  Dorsal  Columns 
Patterns  in  Dorsal  Column  Nuclei 
Patterns  in  Thalamic  Relay  Nucleus 
Definition  of  thalamic  relay  nucleus 
Direct  spinocortical  and  bulbocortical  pathways 
Ipsilateral  pathway  from  dorsal  column  nuclei  to  ventro- 
basal  complex 


Patterns  in  tactile  thalamic  area 
Patterns  in  Postcentral  Homologue  of  Cerebral  Cortex 
Modality  Components  of  Medial  Lemniscal  System 
Touch-Pressure 

Adaptive  properties  of  receptors  and  of  central  neurons 
Peripheral  recepti\'e  fields 

Projection  of  peripheral  receptive  fields  upon  central  neu- 
rons 
Response  Patterns  of  Neurons  of  Medial  Lemniscal  System 
Repetitiveness  of  response  to  single  stimulus 
Response  of  system  to  single  stimulus 
Response  to  two  stimuli  at  different  intervals 
Response  to  repetitive  stimuli  at  diflerent  frequencies 
Afferent  inhibition 
Summary 
Kinesthesis  or  Sense  of  Position  and  Movement  of  Joints 
Muscle  Stretch  Receptors  and  Kinesthesis 
Innervation  of  Joints 

Joint  Receptors  and  Their  Discharge  Patterns 
Central  Projection  of  Joint  Aflferents 
Projections  of  Deep  Receptors  Other  Than  in  Joints 
Functional  Organization  of  First  Somatic  Cortical  Field 
Spinothalamic  System 

Location  of  Tactile  Fibers  in  Spinothalamic  System 
Origin  of  Spinothalamic  System 
Termination  of  Spinothalamic  System 
Topical  Organization  of  Spinothalamic  System 
Ipsilateral  Pathways  of  Spinothalamic  System 
Some  Further  Observations  on  Somatic  Sensory  System 
Relaying  of  Somatic  Afferent  Impulses 
Centrifugal    Pathways    Impinging    LTpon    Sensory    Somatic 

Synaptic  Regions 
Activation  of  Brain  Stem  Reticular  Formation  by  Sensory 

Somatic  Discharges 
Cortical  Fields  Other  Than  Primary  Receiving  Area  Which 
Are  Activated  by  Tactile  Stimuli 
Anatomical  evidence 
Electrophysiological  evidence 
Experimental  psychological  esidence 
Concluding  Remarks 


387 


388 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


INTRODUCTION 

WE  SHALL  DEAL  IN  THIS  CHAPTER  With  neural  events 
which  occur  in  the  nervous  system  in  response  to  me- 
chanical stimulation  of  the  skin  and  of  some  tissues 
beneath  it.  We  shall  focus  our  attention  on  those 
events  which  presumably  provide  the  substrate  for 
tactile  and  kinesthetic  sensations. 

In  the  past  it  was  possible  to  study  such  sensations 
only  by  relating  the  introspective  report  of  a  human 
observer  to  the  experimental  manipulations  at  the 
periphery.  In  this  way  a  large  body  of  psychophysical 
data  was  gathered.  The  events  which  take  place  in 
the  nervous  system  between  the  time  an  appropriate 
stimulus  is  applied  to  the  receptive  zone  and  the  time 
sensation  is  reported  were  not  accessible  to  systematic 
experimentation,  and  virtually  all  the  knowledge 
about  them  was  derived  from  clinical  neurological 
and  neurosurgical  observ-ations. 

Even  though  an  introspective  report  is  still  the  only 
way  Ijy  which  sensations  can  be  studied  directly,  a 
considerable  body  of  electrophysiological  data,  relat- 
ing the  tactile  and  kinesthetic  stimuli  to  the  response 
in  the  central  nervous  system,  has  been  collected  in 
the  past  three  decades.  Perhaps  because  such  data  do 
not  permit,  at  present,  an  interpretation  of  sensations 
in  terms  of  neural  events,  their  impact  on  theory  has 
been  quite  modest.  Even  in  recent  accounts  no  need 
is  felt  to  deal  in  any  detail  with  the  neural  impulses 
farther  centrally  than  the  peripheral  nerve.  Since  it  is 
reasonable  to  believe  that  a  detailed  knowledge  of 
central  neural  events  is  a  prerequisite  for  any  sound 
approach  to  the  problem  of  sensations,  we  shall  deal 
with  the  material  pertaining  to  our  subject  in  a  some- 
what unorthodox  manner.  Thus,  we  shall  not  review 
the  formidable  body  of  psychophysical  data  since  such 
data  are  readily  available  in  all  texts  of  experimental 
psychology.  Likewise,  we  shall  deal  with  the  mor- 
phology of  the  skin  receptors  only  to  the  extent  which 
is  necessary  for  our  purposes.  We  refer  the  interested 
reader  to  a  recent  review  (268)  which  contains  an  ex- 
tensive list  of  literature  on  this  subject.  On  the  other 
hand,  we  shall  consider  the  electrophysiological  data 
pertaining  to  the  receptors  and  the  peripheral  afferent 
fibers  and  shall  deal  with  the  morphology  and  elec- 
trical activity  of  the  central  pathways  and  synaptic 
regions  which  appear  relevant  for  tactile  and  kines- 
thetic sensations. 

We  propose  to  consider  these  sensations  together 
since  the  available  evidence  indicates  that  the  afferent 
impulses  evoked  by  stimulation  of  skin  and  joints  are 
handled  in  the  central   ncr\ous  svstcin  in  a  similar 


fashion  and  in  the  same  synaptic  regions.  By  kines- 
thetic sensations  we  understand  the  appreciation  of 
movement  and  position  of  the  joints.  We  shall  use  the 
term  '  kinesthetic'  which  is  current  in  the  literature  of 
experimental  psychology  instead  of  the  term  *  proprio- 
ceptive' which  was  introduced  by  Sherrington  and 
which  is  almost  universally  used  in  physiological  texts. 
For  once  it  appears  that  a  Sherringtonian  concept 
tended  to  obscure  rather  than  clarify  the  issues.  It  was 
already  established  by  Goldscheider  (99)  that  ap- 
preciation of  movement  of  the  limbs  derives  essen- 
tially from  stimulation  of  the  joints  rather  than 
muscles.  In  harmony  with  these  findings  there  is,  in 
our  opinion,  no  evidence  for  and  strong  evidence 
against  the  notion  that  impulses  provoked  by  stretch 
receptors  in  muscles  provide  information  for  percep- 
tion of  mo\ement  or  position  of  the  joints.  Thus,  it  ap- 
pears that  classical  proprioceptors  may  not  contribute 
at  all  to  the  arousal  of  'proprioceptive'  sensations. 
Hence,  the  more  neutral  term  of  kinesthesis  has  been 
adopted. 

Definitions 

Since  it  is  desirable  to  utilize  electrophysiological 
data  from  animal  experimentations  in  describing 
events  leading  to  tactile  and  kinesthetic  sensations,  it 
is  useful  to  consider  such  sensations  in  terms  of  the 
stimuli  which  provoke  them  and  to  assume  that  in 
mammals,  other  than  man,  comparable  sensations 
arise  when  similar  stimuli  are  applied.  Three  diffi- 
culties arise  in  this  connection.  First,  the  stimuli 
cannot  usually  \x  related  to  the  receptors  themselves, 
as  would  be  desirable,  but  must  be  related  to  the  tissues 
containing  them;  second,  for  the  time  being  neural 
events  cannot  be  related  in  any  simple  way  to  sensa- 
tions; and  third,  not  every  activity  in  the  central 
nerv'ous  system  evoked  by  tactile  and  kinesthetic 
stimuli  necessarily  has  a  bearing  on  the  arousal  of 
sensations. 

From  the  electrophysiological  point  of  view  then, 
one  can  speak  in  a  strict  sense  only  of  the  electrical 
signs  of  neural  activity  aroused  by  tactile  or  kines- 
thetic stimuli.  We  shall  speak,  however,  specifically  of 
tactile  or  kinesthetic  activity  if  the  stimuli  evoke 
responses  in  the  direct  corticopetal  pathways  and  ap- 
propriate synaptic  regions  since  it  seems  fair  to  infer 
that  at  least  this  activity  must  be  instrumental  for  the 
arousal  of  the  appropriate  sensations. 

TACTILE  STIMULI.  We  shall  consider  as  tactile  stimuli 
all   those  which  cause  displacement  of  hairs  or  de- 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


389 


formation  of  skin  without  injury.  Since  most  of  the 
experimental  data  pertain  to  animals,  it  is  usually  not 
practical  to  distinguish  between  stimuli  which  cause 
sensations  of  touch  from  those  of  light  pressure. 

KINESTHETIC  STIMULI.  Stimuli  pressing  upon  or  dis- 
placing without  injury  the  connective  tissue  under- 
neath the  skin,  periosteum,  bones,  sheaths  of  tendons 
or  muscle  fascia,  and  capsules  of  the  joints  lead  to  sen- 
sations often  referred  to  as  deep  sensibility.  We  shall 
be  concerned  in  particular  with  those  stimuli  which 
cause  displacement  or  compression  of  joint  capsules 
and  shall  refer  to  them  as  kinesthetic.  Under  physio- 
logical conditions,  of  course,  it  is  the  contraction  of 
the  muscle  which  acts  as  a  major  kinesthetic  stimulus. 
This  fact,  however,  has  no  bearing  on  the  assertion 
which  is  justified  on  page  410  that  stretch  receptors  in 
the  muscle  itself  play  no  direct  role  in  the  arousal  of 
kinesthetic  sensations. 

Electrophysiological  Methods  Jor  Study  of  Somatic 
Afferent  Systems 

When  a  light  tactile  stimulus  is  delivered  to  a  small 
area  on  the  body  .surface,  it  evokes  a  burst  of  impulses 
in  afferent  nerve  fibers.  This  volley  is  relayed  through 
afferent  pathways  and  synaptic  regions  of  the  spinal 
cord,  brain  stem  and  thalamus,  and  invades  the 
sensory  areas  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  An  electrode 
placed  at  .some  point  in  the  system  samples  the  elec- 
trical signs  of  this  ev^oked  activity  and  provides  a  tool 
for  study  of  its  whereabouts  and  nature  and  of  the 
patterning  of  the  central  reflection  of  the  body  form. 
The  variables  of  the  experiment  are  the  form  of  stimu- 
lation used,  the  state  of  excitability  of  the  neurons 
and  the  method  of  recording.  Each  deserves  a  com- 
ment. 

METHODS  OF  STIMULATION.  The  somatic  afferent  system 
presents  difficulties  for  experimental  study  for  only 
exceptionally  has  it  so  far  been  possible  to  deliver 
quantitatively  precise  stimuli  such  as  those  available 
for  activation  of  the  auditory  and  visual  systems. 
Various  mechanical  devices  for  quick  displacement  of 
hairs  or  skin  or  for  rotation  of  joints  are  commonly 
used,  but  few  of  them  provide  a  wide  range  of  action. 
This  had  led  many  investigators  to  resort  to  electrical 
stimulation  of  nerve  trunks  to  achieve  an  exact 
temporal  positioning  and  pattern  of  the  stimuli.  It 
was  often  believed  that  a  dependable  correlation  exists 
between  certain  groups  of  fibers,  separable  by  stimu- 
lus  strength,    and    certain    modalities   of  sensations. 


However,  this  correlation  is  far  from  exact,  and  it  can 
hardly  be  doubted  that  the  use  of  massive  nerve  \ol- 
leys  has  led  occasionally  to  conclusions  of  question- 
able physiological  significance.  The  large  electrical 
fields  created  around  massive  volleys  traversing  the 
central  nervous  system  are  likely  also  to  lead  to 
ephaptic  excitation  of  neurons  of  perhaps  completely 
unrelated  function.  Electrical  stimuli  deliv^ered  within 
or  across  the  skin  allow  a  topographical  positioning 
of  the  stimulus,  but  since  the  volley  e\'oked  is  a  svn- 
chronous  one  it  cannot,  of  course,  be  easily  compared 
with  that  produced  Ijy  a  natural  stimulus.  A  new  ad- 
vance in  stimulation  technique  is  badly  needed. 

EXCITABILITY  STATES  OF  CENTRAL  NEURONS.  The  re- 
markable safety  factor  of  synaptic  transmission  at 
relays  of  at  least  some  corticopetal  tactile  and  kines- 
thetic pathways  renders  them  in  certain  aspects  rela- 
tively immune  to  the  depressing  effects  of  anesthetic 
agents.  By  contrast  the  activity  evoked  by  tactile  and 
kinesthetic  stimuli  in  systems  which  do  not  conduct 
towards  the  cortex  and  therefore  are  not  likely  to  con- 
tribute directly  to  conscious  perception  are  extra- 
ordinarily susceptible  to  these  depressing  effects.  This 
differential  is  of  great  advantage  for  it  allows  a  de- 
tailed mapping  of  the  place  and  patterns  of  the 
central  projection  of  the  sensory  surface.  However, 
the  temporal  capacity  of  the  corticopetal  systems  for 
transmission  as  well  as  certain  other  functional  char- 
acteristics are  severely  depressed  by  barbiturates. 

The  desire  to  retain  a  high  level  of  excitability  in 
an  anesthetized  animal  has  led  many  investigators  to 
use  chloralose  as  an  anesthetic  agent,  frequently  com- 
bined with  a  neuromuscular  blocking  drug.  While 
connections  revealed  under  these  conditions  un- 
doubtedly exist,  the  abnormal  excitability  of  the 
brain  calls  for  particular  caution  in  evaluating  the 
findings  obtained.  Under  these  conditions  the  trans- 
mission capacity  revealed  is  as  seriously  abnormal  in 
one  direction  as  it  is  in  the  other  under  deep  bar- 
biturate narcosis.  Recently  a  combination  of  light 
barbiturate  narcosis  with  neuromuscular  blocking 
agents  has  allowed  a  somewhat  closer  approach  to  the 
normal  state.  An  important  advance  has  been  made 
by  Bremer  (30,  31)  by  introducing  the  encephale  isole 
preparation,  although  the  high  cervical  transection 
makes  such  a  preparation  suitable  for  study  of  only 
certain  sensory  somatic  problems.  On  the  other  hand, 
many  investigators  resort  to  the  use  of  unanesthetized 
animals  held  motionless  by  neuromuscular  blocking 
drugs.  The  latter  method,  apart  from  the  hesitations 
one   may   ha\e   in   using  such   preparations,   hardly 


390 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


offers  a  solution  of  the  problem  for  the  neuromuscu- 
lar blocking  agents  themselves  have  powerful  effects 
upon  central  synaptic  transmission.  For  this  reason 
the  method  of  recording  from  electrodes  chronically 
implanted  in  the  brain  or  upon  its  surface  is  now  fre- 
quently employed.  Recovery  of  the  animals  from 
the  anesthetic  allows  the  study  of  evoked  electrical 
activity  for  periods  of  weeks  or  months  under  condi- 
tions closely  approaching  the  normal.  The  developing 
technique  of  recording  from  single  units  by  means  of 
implanted  microelectrodes  promises  to  be  fruitful. 

METHODS  OF  RECORDING.  An  analysis  of  the  electrical 
signs  of  neural  activity  evoked  in  the  central  nervous 
system  by  sensory  stimuli  is  treated  extensively  in 
Chapters  X  and  XII  of  this  volume.  Here  we  merely 
wish  to  point  out  that  it  is  the  initially  positive  slow 
wave  which  has  proved  of  great  value,  particularly  in 
the  experiments  designed  to  determine  the  locations 
of  the  responsive  regions  and  the  pattern  of  the 
sensory  projection.  The  usefulness  of  this  response  is 
hardly  minimized  by  the  fact  that  its  nature  still  re- 
mains obscure. 

The  method  of  single  unit  analysis  introduced  by 
Adrian  precipitated  a  rapid  advance  in  neurophysi- 
ology. So  useful  has  it  become  that  an  appreciable 
share  of  present  day  research  is  based  upon  it.  Both 
intra-  and  extracellular  microelectrodes  are  commonly 
used.  Successful  application  of  the  extracellular 
method  requires  that  the  unit  observed  be  held  under 
study  for  considerable  periods  of  time  in  a  relatively 
undamaged  state.  The  method  permits  determination 
of  response  properties  and  topographic  and  modality 
attributes  of  a  sample  of  cells  at  a  given  location  which 
allows  a  reconstruction  of  the  behavior  of  the  popula- 
tion. A  full  reconstruction,  however,  would  be  possible 
only  if  the  sample  were  sufhciently  large  and  unbiased. 
This  last  requirement  is  proljably  not  met,  for  it  seems 
most  likely  that  the  smaller  elements  of  the  neural 
population  are  in  fact  rejected  by  the  recording  instru- 
ments presently  available. 


CURRENT  THEORIES   OF   CUT.^NEOUS  SENS.^TIONS 

Classic  Concept 

According  to  the  concepts  developed  by  von  Frey 
(244-247)  pain,  cold,  warmth  and  touch  represent  the 
four  basic  modalities  of  cutaneous  sensation  and  spe- 
cific receptors  can  be  assigned  to  each  modality.  The 
older  anatomical  and  psychophysical  findings  were 


generally  interpreted  in  the  light  of  these  notions  and 
unqualified  support  of  the  orthodox  view  was  given 
by  the  work  of  VVoollard  et  al.  (275),  and  earlier  work 
by  Weddell  (264,  265)  and  VVeddell  &  Harpman 
(266).  The  views  of  von  Frey,  however,  did  not  remain 
altogether  unchallenged  and  two  formulations  differ- 
ing greatly  from  the  classical  concepts  will  be  briefly 
considered. 

Pattern  Thcnry 

Recently  the  Oxford  group  of  workers  (113,  154, 
155,  220,  223,  267-269)  seems  to  deny  altogether  the 
existence  of  modality  specific  receptors.  The  conclu- 
sion of  the  group  is  that  different  cutaneous  sensations 
arise  not  as  a  result  of  selective  activation  of  specific 
receptors  but  because  different  stimuli  affect  the  same 
sets  of  fibers  in  a  different  manner.  Thus,  in  the  first 
order  neurons  different  discharge  patterns  in  the 
same  fiber  bundle,  and  not  a  selective  activation  of 
some  fibers  in  the  bundle,  are  thought  to  determine 
the  different  cutaneous  sensations.  The  reasons  for 
this  deduction  are  that  these  workers  were  unable  to 
relate  specified  endings  to  specific  modalities  in  sev- 
eral skin  areas  (113,  155,  223)  and  that  all  modalities 
of  sensations  were  obtained  by  stimulating  the  cornea 
(154)  which  is  known  to  have  free  endings  only. 
Moreover,  histological  evidence  led  them  to  believe 
that  all  endings  in  the  skin  are  essentially  alike  since 
all  arborize  into  fine,  naked,  axoplasmic  filaments. 
They  further  conclude  that  a  classification  of  en- 
capsulated endings  into  various  types  is  untenable 
since  a  large  number  of  morphologically  intermediate 
variants  exists  between  the  usually  recognized  types. 

We  believe  that  the  Oxford  workers  did  produce 
suggestive  evidence  that  stimulation  of  free  endings 
may  cause  sensations  which  can  be  classified  in  the 
broad  spectrum  of  touch.  Such  findings  however  do 
not  at  all  establish  that  specific  endings  do  not  exist, 
and  the  conclusions  drawn  from  histological  observa- 
tions do  not  appear  convincing.  If  a  crisis  exists  in 
respect  to  evaluating  the  morphology  of  the  endings, 
it  is  a  crisis  of  abundance  and  not  of  scarcity.  One 
hesitates  to  accept  as  a  solution  to  the  vexing  problem 
of  the  morphology  of  the  encapsulated  endings  a 
declaration  that  virtually  all  morphological  differ- 
ences between  them  are  either  insignificant  or  are  due 
to  artifacts  of  the  technique. 

In  any  case,  a  further  elaboration  of  the  idea  of  the 
discharge  patterns  led  at  least  some  workers  of  the 
Oxford  group  to  opinions  about  touch  which  are 
closer   to   classical   notions   than   one   would  expect. 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


391 


Thus,  Weddell  el  al.  (267)  conclude  that  the  ana- 
tomical arrangement  of  the  axoplasmic  filaments  of 
the  encapsulated  endings  is  such  that  one  could  expect 
them — in  contrast  to  the  free  endings — to  be  highly 
sensitive  to  minimal  deformations.  A  differential 
sensitivity  to  mechanical  deformation,  however,  is  all 
that  could  reasonably  be  required  to  declare  such  an 
ending  as  specific  for  touch  or  pressure.  The  differ- 
ence, then,  between  the  Oxford  authors  and  the 
orthodox  view  in  this  instance  reduces  itself  to  the 
proposition  that  the  Oxford  workers  presumably  as- 
sume that  discharge  in  some  other  fibers  as  well  must 
necessarily  occur  before  a  touch  sensation  is  recog- 
nized while  we  are  inclined  to  think,  with  von  Frey, 
that  in  principle  such  a  sensation  could  arise  if  a  single 
appropriate  peripheral  fiber  were  activated. 

Concept  of  Head 

An  important  difficulty  in  drawing  conclusions 
from  psychophysical  experiments  is  the  uncertainty 
about  the  classifying  of  some  sensations  which  may- 
still  be  called  touch.  This  uncertainty  is  clearly  at  the 
root  of  the  controversy  as  to  whether  or  not  stimula- 
tion of  the  free  endings  actually  arouses  tactile  sensa- 
tions. The  interpretation  of  von  Frey  denies  that  this 
is  the  case  and  considers  such  sensations  as  akin  to 
pain;  the  O.xford  interpretation  affirms  the  tactile 
quality  of  such  sensations  and  denies  the  existence  of 
specific  receptors.  Head  and  his  collaborators  (118) 
proposed  that  there  are  basically  two  different  kinds 
of  sensations  subserved  by  a  dual  sensory  mechanism 
at  the  periphery,  the  more  generalized  and,  as  they 
felt,  more  primitive  or  protopathic  type  and  the  more 
specific  and  advanced  or  epicritic  system.  The  idea  of 
the  duality  of  cutaneous  sensations  was  greatly  elab- 
orated by  Head  and  this  elaboration  might  have  con- 
tributed to  the  present  eclipse  of  his  concepts.  Al- 
though at  first  accepted  by  many,  they  were  soon 
sharply  criticized,  and  finally  Walshe  (262)  in  his 
review  of  the  subject  delivered  a  cou[)  de  grace  to  these 
concepts  by  pointing  out  that  the  crucial  introspective 
experimental  observations  of  Head  were  not  inter- 
preted in  the  same  way  by  any  of  the  suljsequent 
observers  and  that  his  theoretical  elaboration  was 
sometimes  hazy  and  contradictory  in  details,  often 
incompatible  with  the  present  knowledge  and  always 
speculative.  However  much  one  may  disagree  with 
some  of  Walshe' s  criticisms  the  fact  remains  that  none 
of  the  experimental  observations  offered  by  Head  in 
support  of  his  ideas  has  been  accepted  by  others. 

The  conclusion  that  Head  did  not  prove  his  point 


is,  however,  irrelevant  for  an  inquiry  as  to  whether  or 
not  his  central  idea  has  merit.  It  is  apparent  that  a 
protopathic  system,  if  it  exists,  is  likely  to  be  repre- 
sented anatomically  by  free  endings.  The  question 
may  then  be  asked  whether  stimulation  of  such  end- 
ings results  in  sensations  other  than  pain.  There  are 
some  indications  that  this  indeed  may  be  the  case. 
Evidence  to  this  effect  seems  to  be  at  present  the  chief 
support  for  the  pattern  theory  of  the  Oxford  workers, 
even  though  such  findings  could  argue,  in  better 
harmony  with  other  well  established  facts,  for  the 
existence  of  protopathic  .sensibility. 

Another  hint  that  a  protopathic  system  may  exist 
is  offered  by  studies  of  action  potentials  in  peripheral 
nerve.  We  shall  discuss  these  in  some  detail  later.  Here 
it  is  sufficient  to  state  that  there  is  evidence  that  im- 
pulses evoked  by  tactile  stimuli  are  conducted  in  both 
myelinated  and  unmyelinated  fibers.  Finally,  the  fact 
that  activity  aroused  by  tactile  stimuli  is  conducted 
within  the  spinal  cord  in  at  least  two  independent 
ascending  pathways  could  be  utilized  to  argue  that 
the  idea  of  duality  of  the  tactile  system  does  not  appear 
unreasonable. 

It  should  be  apparent  from  these  remarks  that  it  is 
not  proposed  at  present  to  revive  the  concepts  of  epi- 
critic and  protopathic  sensibility.  We  wish  only  to 
point  out  that  an  unqualified  rejection  of  these  con- 
cepts may  be  premature  and  that  Head's  ideas  in  some 
form  may  yet  prove  useful  in  the  future. 


SOME    PROPERTIES    OF    PERIPHERAL    SOMATIC 
AFFERENT  SYSTEM 

Receptors 

It  is  convenient  to  consider  the  receptors  both  in 
this  section  and  in  the  one  which  deals  with  the  cen- 
tral events.  In  order  to  avoid  repetition  we  shall  con- 
sider here  primarilv  those  properties  which  have  a 
bearing  on  their  specificity. 

SPECIFICITY  OF  RECEPTORS.  Whatever  opinions  one 
may  hold  about  the  way  tactile  stimuli  arouse  sensa- 
tions it  is  fundamental  to  recognize  that  there  are 
some  receptors  which  are  specifically  sensitive  to  such 
stimuli.  Conclusive  evidence  in  this  respect  is  provided 
by  studies  of  discharges,  usually  of  single  units,  when 
mechanoreceptors  are  activated  by  natural  stimuli. 
In  many  of  these  studies  (4-6,  16-18,  29,  42,  68,  94, 
124,  127,  161,  187)  the  existence  of  a  specific  receptor 
is  inferred  from  the  behavior  of  the  neural  discharge; 


392 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


in  some  (12,  105-108),  the  receptors  themsehes  were 
identified. 

It  was  observed  early  and  since  confirmed  by  all 
workers  in  the  field  that  the  largest  fibers  in  the  cu- 
taneous nerves  are  activated  by  certain  types  of 
tactile  stimuli.  It  may  be  stressed  immediately  that 
the  converse  statement  does  not  hold  and  that  ac- 
ti\ity  aroused  by  tactile  stimuli  i>  nut  limited  to  large 
fibers  only.  Since  for  technical  reasons  discharges 
occurring  in  large  fibers  are  particularily  convenient 
to  study,  the  information  given  below  regarding 
tactile  receptors  pertains  to  those  which  are  con- 
nected to  such  fibers. 

The  specificity  of  tactile  receptors  manifests  itself 
both  in  their  exquisite  sensitivity  to  mechanical 
stimuli  and  in  their  lack  of  sensitivity  or  high  threshold 
for  other  than  such  stimuli.  The  threshold  to  mechan- 
ical stimuli  is  low,  apparentlv  commensurate  with  the 
capacitv  of  the  animal  to  recognize  them,  and  the 
details  of  mechanical  application  of  the  stimulus  are 
as  a  rule  critical  for  evoking  discharges.  Direct  meas- 
urements made  on  the  mesenteric  Pacinian  cor- 
puscles (105)  indicate  that  the  minimal  movement  of 
the  stimulus  probe  necessary  to  excite  the  receptor  is 
of  the  order  of  0.5  m  in  too  /xsec;  the  Pacinian  cor- 
puscle in  the  toe  (107)  showed  a  similar  sensitivity. 
There  are  no  figures  available  for  any  other  ending. 
From  qualitative  observations,  though,  there  is  no 
reason  to  doubt  that  at  least  a  number  of  them  is 
equally  sensitive. 

The  problem  of  sensitivity  of  mechanoreceptors  to 
other  than  tactile  stimuli  has  attracted  but  casual 
interest  of  most  observers,  chiefly  because  it  is  known 
that  thermal  and  painful  stimuli  characteristically 
cause  discharges  in  the  smaller  spectrum  of  fibers. 
Ne\ertheless,  it  was  observed  by  .\drian  &  Umrath  (5) 
that  thermal  stimuli  did  not  excite  the  mechanorecep- 
tors they  studied,  and  Hogg  (127)  stated  that  thermal 
and  chemical  stimuli  are  less  effectise  in  the  frog  in  ac- 
tivating large  fibers  than  small  ones  and  that  the  re- 
verse is  true  for  tactile  stimuli.  Hensel  &  Zotterman 
(124)  recently  presented  interesting  data  on  the  re- 
sponse of  some  mechanoreceptors  to  cold.  In  the 
tongue  of  the  cat  they  found  mechanoreceptors  not 
sensitive  to  strong  thermal  stimuli  as  well  as  thermo- 
receptors unresponsive  to  tactile  excitation.  In  addi- 
tion to  these  receptors  they  also  found  a  group  of  fibers 
which  responded  both  to  pressure  and  to  cooling.  The 
response  to  cold  differed  in  important  aspects  from  the 
response  of  a  typical  thermoreceptor  for  the  response 
occurred  only  to  ver\  low  temperatures  and  rapid 
cooling  (while  thermoreceptors  respond  with  a  sensi- 


ti\ity  to  about  a  tenth  of  a  degree  below  40°C:)  and  it 
adapted  to  extinction  within  a  few  seconds  (while  a 
typical  response  to  cooling  persists  for  as  long  as  the 
stimulus  is  applied).  How  to  interpret  such  a  response 
to  cooling  is  an  open  question.  It  is  conceivable  that  it 
represents  a  secondary  effect  due,  for  example,  to  a 
displacement  of  a  mechanoreceptor  through  vasocon- 
striction, although  this  interpretation  is  considered  as 
rather  unlikely  by  the  authors. 

TYPES  OF  DiscH.\RGES.  The  mechanorcceptors  in  the 
skin  can  be  divided  into  fast  and  slowly  adapting 
types.  Most  receptors  activated  by  displacement  of 
hairs  are  fast  adapting.  Those  responding  to  pressure 
adapt  either  fast  or  slowly.  It  is  of  interest  that  recep- 
tors activated  by  kinesthetic  stimuli  appear  to  have 
the  same  properties  as  the  mechanoreceptors  in  the 
skin  although  the  slowly  adapting  ones  seem  to  pre- 
dominate greatly  in  numbers  (29,  225). 

It  is  unfortunately  not  known  how  the  morpho- 
logical structure  of  the  ending  relates  to  the  dis- 
charge pattern  since,  except  for  the  Pacinian  cor- 
puscle, none  of  the  other  receptors  has  ever  been 
studied  in  isolation.  For  that  reason  the  significance 
of  the  capsule  and  of  the  accessory  fibers  is  entirely 
obscure.  Recently  Boyd  (28)  and  Skoglund  (225) 
identified  Ruffini's  endings  in  the  joint  capsule  as 
slowly  adapting  receptors  and  the  modified  Pacinian 
corpuscles  as  the  fast  adapting  ones.  These  identifica- 
tions, howe\er,  must  be  considered  tentative  since 
they  are  indirect.  The  Pacinian  corpuscle,  the  best 
known  receptor  at  present,  is  fast  adapting  (107).  It 
is  known  for  this  receptor  (106)  that  its  adaptation  to 
mechanical  stimuli  is  a  property  of  the  receptor  itself 
rather  than  of  its  fiber.  Loewenstein  C161),  working  on 
frog's  skin,  presented  some  data  to  suggest  that  a 
fast  adapting  receptor  may  be  made  to  discharge  for 
a  long  time  if  the  tension  in  the  receptor  region  is 
greatly  increased,  thus  implying  that  whether  an 
ending  adapts  quickly  or  slowly  may  be  determined 
by  the  mechanical  arrangement  of  the  ending.  If  this 
be  so,  the  degree  of  coiling  of  the  terminals  could  be  a 
determining  factor  in  whether  a  receptor  is  fast  or 
slowlv  adapting.  The  merit  of  this  suggestion  'is  at 
present  difficult  to  evaluate. 

RECEPTOR  POTENTi.\L.  As  already  mentioned,  the 
only  receptor  the  functional  properties  of  which 
have  thus  far  been  studied  is  the  Pacinian  corpuscle. 
It  has  been  recently  established  by  Alvarez-Buylla  & 
de  Arellano  (12)  that  mechanical  stimuli  produce  a 
local  response  which  Gray  &  Sato  (108)  propose  to 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


393 


call  a  receptor  potential  since  this  potential — in  con- 
trast to  the  local  response  of  the  nerve — is  not  affected 
by  near  absence  of  sodium.  The  receptor  potential 
can  summate  and  its  amplitude  depends  on  stimulus 
strength.  It  is  set  up  in  less  than  0.2  msec,  and  is  the 
earliest  electrical  event  which  is  known  to  occur. 
While  the  mechanism  of  its  generation  is  at  the  mo- 
ment quite  obscure,  its  occurrence  provides  a  final 
indication — if  such  be  needed — that  the  Pacinian 
corpuscle  must  be  regarded  as  a  full-fledged  receptor. 
An  electronmicroscopical  description  of  its  fairly 
complex  structure  has  been  given  recently  by  Pease  & 
Quilliam  (195).  The  mechanism  of  excitation  of 
Pacinian  corpuscles  is  discussed  by  Gray  in  Chapter 
I\'  of  this  work. 

Peripheral  Cutaneous  Nerve  Fibers 

IMPULSES    IN    PERIPHER.AL    NERVE    FIBERS.    ThuS    far    WC 

have  discussed  only  to  what  extent  discharges  in  single 
units  reflect  some  properties  of  the  receptors  which 
initiate  the  impulses.  We  shall  now  turn  to  electro- 
physiological evidence  derived  from  studies  of  cutane- 
ous nerves.  Since  all  experimental  data  are  derived 
either  from  studies  of  single  units  or  from  studies  of 
the  electroneurogram,  it  will  be  useful  to  recall  some 
properties  of  the  peripheral  fibers. 

As  is  well  known,  a  cutaneous  nerve  consists  of 
fibers  of  different  sizes.  Fibers  of  different  diameters 
do  not,  however,  occur  with  equal  frequency  and 
the  size-frequency  distribution  curve  for  any  cutane- 
ous nerve  shows  characteristically  several  peaks.  It  is 
customary  to  classify  all  fibers  into  A,  B  and  C  groups 
according  to  certain  characteristics  of  their  action 
potentials  which  are  different  for  each  of  them.  How- 
ever, it  is  unnecessary  to  relate  here  these  character- 
istics since  in  the  cutaneous  nerves  only  A  and  C 
fibers  are  known  and  the  myelinated  and  unmye- 
linated fibers  form  the  A  and  C  groups,  respectively. 

It  is  known  that  the  velocity  of  conduction  in  A 
fibers  varies  with  their  diameters.  If  an  as.sumption  is 
made  that  the  amplitude  of  the  action  potential  as 
recorded  across  the  membrane  is  about  the  same  for 
any  A  fiber  but  that  the  height  of  the  externally  re- 
corded potential  varies  as  the  square  of  the  diameter 
of  the  fiber,  it  is  possible  to  estimate  the  size  of  the 
fiber  from  the  externally  recorded  amplitude  of  the 
discharge  in  a  single  unit  preparation.  Making  these 
assumptions  for  the  entire  nerve  the  shape  of  the  com- 
pound action  potential  can  be  reconstructed  with 
great  accuracy  if  the  fiber  composition  of  the  nerve  is 
known.  Conversely,  it  is  justifiable  to  infer  from  the 


different  elevations  of  the  electroneurogram  the 
presence  of  fiber  groups  of  specified  diameters  in  a 
given  nerve.  Usually,  Greek  letters  are  used  to  denote 
the  different  elevations,  each  successiv-e  letter  referring 
to  a  group  of  fibers  of  smaller  diameter  (fig.  i).  Some 
confusion  resulted  occasionally  in  the  past  with  the 
use  of  this  notation,  for  different  nerves  do  in  fact 
differ  in  their  fiber  compositon  and  thus  an  elevation 
denoted  by  the  same  letter  in  different  nerves  may 
indicate  at  least  somewhat  different  fiber  groups.  For 
the  corresponding  nerves  in  the  same  species  the  dis- 
tribution of  fibers  is,  according  to  Gasser  (88),  quite 
constant. 

IMPULSES    EVOKED     IN     FIBERS    OF    DIFFERENT    SIZE     BY 

T.-iiCTiLE  STIMULI.  All  workers  agree  that  tactile  stimuli 
activate  the  largest  fibers  in  the  cutaneous  nerves  and 
those  who  distinguish  gentle  contact  (touch)  from 
sustained  displacement  (pressure)  invariably  state 
that  it  is  a  gentle  contact  which  activates  the  largest 
fibers.  Maruhashi  et  al.  (168)  report  that  the  diam- 
eters of  the  fibers  activated  hy  touch  vary  between  8 
to  14  /J  in  the  cat  and  8  to  15  |i  in  the  frog.  They 
further  found  that  movements  of  hairs  activate  fibers 
in  the  range  of  6  to   1 2  /i,  while  diameters  of  fibers 


FIG.  1.  Compound  action  potential  of  the  saphenous  nerve 
of  the  cat  recorded  at  a  distance  of  54  mm  from  the  locus  of 
stimulation.  Several  elevations  (denoted  by  Greek  letters)  are 
recorded  because,  in  the  nerve,  fibers  of  different  sizes  are 
giouped  around  several  peaks.  Since  the  saphenous  nerve  lacks 
the  largest  afferents  arising  from  the  muscle  stretch  receptors 
no  a  elevation  is  indicated  even  though  the  sizes  in  the  a  and 
(3  groups  overlap.  The  /3  and  7  elevations  as  denoted  here  are 
sometimes  referred  to  as  a  and  0  peaks.  All  elevations  pertain  to 
\  fibers.  The  elevation  due  to  C  fibers  is  not  shown.  Time 
line:  5,000  cps. 


394 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HYSIOLOrn- 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


excited  by  pressure  are  3  to  5  /i  in  the  cat  and  4  to  5  m 
in  the  frog.  In  addition,  unmyelinated  fibers  of  the  C 
group  were  seen  by  them  to  be  activated  by  mechan- 
ical stimuli  both  in  the  cat  and  frog. 

The  findings  of  the  Japanese  observers  confirm  the 
older  observations  of  Zotterman  (282)  who,  recording 
from  strands  of  the  saphenous  nerve  of  the  cat,  noted 
that,  apart  from  the  usual  discharges  evoked  by 
tactile  stimuli  in  the  large  fibers,  discharges  were 
evoked  in  smaller  fibers  also  by  very  light  stroking  of 
the  skin.  These  potentials  contribute  to  the  delta  ele- 
vation of  the  electroneurogram  of  the  saphenous 
nerve  and  Zotterman  judged  the  appropriate  fibers  to 
be  in  the  range  of  5  to  g  n.  He  also  noticed  that  with 
stroking  of  the  skin  the  delta  potentials  are  followed 
by  a  ca.scade  of  very  small  spikes  which  he  felt  must 
be  conducted  by  the  C  group  of  fillers. 

REL.'^TION     OF     CUT.'>iNEOUS     STIMULI     TO     ACTIVITY     IN 

FIBERS  OF  DIFFERENT  SIZE.  Observations  on  single  units 
indicate  then  that  tactile  stimuli  can  activate  at  least 
several  groups  of  afi'erent  fibers  and,  if  the  data  of  the 
Japanese  workers  are  taken  as  a  basis,  the  conclusion 
seems  inescapable  that  no  fiber  whatever  within 
either  the  A  or  C  group  can  be  eliminated  by  virtue 
of  its  size  alone  as  potentially  responding  to  tactile 
stimuli.  Nevertheless,  the  data  imply  that  a  relation 
may  exist  between  the  size  of  a  fiber  and  the  exact 
quality  of  the  mechanical  stimulus  which  activates  it. 
The  conclusion  that  fibers  of  all  sizes  may  be  activated 
by  mechanical  stimuli  must  not  imply  that  all  fibers 
in  the  cutaneous  nerve  can  Ije  activated  by  them.  If 
this  were  so,  each  fiber  activated  by  other  than  me- 
chanical stimuli  would  also  be  responsive  to  tactile 
stimulation.  This  is  apparently  not  the  case  since  at 
least  some  fibers  which  are  selectively  activated  by 
thermal  stimuli  have  been  shown  to  exist  (120-123, 
281). 

The  problem  of  C  fibers  is  of  special  interest.  There 
is  evidence  available  (22,  46,  149,  168,  282)  that 
nociceptive  stimuli  can  activate  such  fibers.  From  the 
experiments  in  which  C  fiber  activity  could  be  identi- 
fied with  certainty  it  has  been  inferred  (46)  that  at 
least  some  thermal  stimuli  (warmth)  can  also  cause  C 
fiber  activity.  All  workers  who  used  single  unit 
preparations  and  were  concerned  with  this  question 
(127,  168,  282)  have  concluded  that  not  only  nocicep- 
tive but  cold,  warmth  and  mechanical  stimuli  acti- 
vate C  fibers  as  well.  It  should  lie  noted  that  with  the 
single  fiber  technique  it  may  not  always  be  possible 
to  decide  that  a  C  fiber  and  not  a  small  A  fiber  has 
been  activated.   Nevertheless,   the   evidence  suggests 


indeed  that  C  fibers  can  be  acti\ated  by  all  modes  of 
cutaneous  stimulation. 

In  contrast  to  the  findings  about  A  fibers  there  is 
no  conclusive  evidence,  either  for  or  against,  concern- 
ing selective  sensitivity  of  single  C  fibers  to  various 
stimuli.  It  thus  remains  an  open  question  to  what 
extent  the  C  fibers  resemble  the  A  system. 

RELATION    OF   ELEV.\TIONS   OF   ELECTRONEUROGRAM    TO 

MODALITIES  OF  SENSATION.  The  findings  derived  from 
observations  of  single  units  seem  to  agree  with  the 
studies  which  relate  the  different  elevations  of  the 
compound  action  potential  to  the  results  of  psycho- 
physical and  animal  experiments  in  which  the  periph- 
eral nerve  is  blocked  by  infiltration  with  local  anes- 
thetic, made  ischemic  or  excited  by  electrical 
stimuli. 

It  has  long  been  known  for  man  that  perineural 
injection  of  cocaine  (or  a  similarly  acting  agent) 
blocks  sensations  in  a  preferential  order  in  such  a 
way  that  cold,  warmth,  pain  and  touch  disappear  in 
the  order  stated.  There  is  some  discrepancy  among 
various  observers  whether  it  is  cold  or  pain  which 
disappears  first,  but  there  is  an  almost  unanimous 
agreement  that  it  is  touch  which  disappears  last.  [For 
some  discordant  oljservations  and  a  review  of  the 
literature  .see  Sinclair  &  Hinshaw  (221,  222).] 

Since  Gasser  &  Erlanger  (89)  demonstrated  that 
cocainization  Ijlocks  conduction  in  an  orderly  se- 
quence, the  smallest  fibers  being  blocked  first,  it  can 
be  inferred  that  the  largest  fibers  in  the  nerve  are 
activated  by  tactile  stimuli.  This  conclusion,  of 
course,  is  but  a  confirmation  of  the  firmly  established 
findings  discussed  earlier.  It  should  be  stressed  that 
cocaine  block  does  not  permit  any  conclusions  as  to 
whether  smaller  fibers  which  can  be  activated  by 
touch  exist,  but  it  does  imply  that  activity  in  a  group 
of  the  largest  fibers  alone  may  be  quite  sufficient  for 
the  arousal  of  tactile  sensations.  Direct  stimulation  of 
an  exposed  nerve  in  man  (119)  leads  to  an  identical 
conclusion  since  it  is  possible  to  excite  only  the  largest 
fibers  with  an  appropriate  stimulus  and  since  such 
stimuli  lead  only  to  an  arousal  of  tactile  sensations. 

In  contrast  to  a  block  produced  by  a  local  anes- 
thetic, application  of  pressure  over  a  limb  of  man 
leads  to  disappearance  of  sensations  usually  in  the 
following  order:  touch,  cold,  warmth,  pain.  In  experi- 
ments of  Clark,  Hughes  and  Gasser  as  reported  by 
Gasser  (87),  compression  of  a  limb  of  a  cat  led  first  to 
a  conduction  failure  of  the  delta  fibers  and  of  the 
largest  fibers  in  the  ner\e.  The  exact  prosjre.ss  of  the 
conduction  failure  was  difficult  to  establish,  but  it  was 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


395 


clear  that  the  failure  did  not  follow  an  orderly  se- 
quence according  to  fiber  size.  The  important  finding 
was  that  even  after  the  entire  spectrum  of  A  fibers 
failed  to  conduct,  the  C  elevation  was  only  little  im- 
paired. It  can  thus  be  concluded  that  C  fibers  are 
more  resistant  to  ischemia  than  is  the  A  group  and 
since  manifestations  of  painful  sensations  are  still 
evokable  when  only  C  fibers  conduct,  one  can  infer 
that  painful  stimuli  must  activate  at  least  some  C 
fibers.  While  this  finding  again  agrees  with  what  has 
been  more  recently  shown  by  other  methods,  one  can 
conclude  in  addition  that  activity  in  C  fibers  alone  is 
apparently  sufficient  to  arouse  painful  sensations.  It 
could  have  been  expected  perhaps  that  some  tactile 
sensations  should  be  present  as  long  as  C  fibers  are 
conducting  if  it  be  true  that  mechanical  stimuli  excite 
such  fibers.  The  negative  findings  may  mean,  of 
course,  that  there  are  no  C  fibers  activated  by  touch. 
It  may  mean  as  well  that  activity  in  C  fibers  aroused 
by  tactile  stimuli  under  the  experimental  conditions 
tested  are  not  interpreted  as  touch,  or  finally  even  that 
some  perhaps  obscure  qualities  of  tactile  sensations 
which  actually  were  present  were  ignored  by  the  ex- 
perimenters and  the  experimental  subjects  alike. 

SUMMARY.  It  appears  that  the  availaljle  neurophysio- 
logical  evidence  in  respect  to  the  peripheral  aspects 
of  the  tactile  system  does  not  support  fully  any  of  the 
current  ideas  regarding  tactile  sensations. 

Despite  the  arguments  advanced  by  the  Oxford 
workers  the  evidence  seems  conclusive  that  there 
e.xist  in  fact  specific  tactile  (as  well  as  thermal)  recep- 
tors, The  evidence  is  also  good  that  the  fiber  size  may 
be  indicative  of  connections  with  some  specific  recep- 
tors. Thus,  the  known  thermoreceptors  seem  con- 
nected with  small  or  medium  sized  fibers  only,  while 
the  largest  fibers  in  the  cutaneous  nerve  are  con- 
nected to  mechanoreceptors.  Bishop  (21}  points  out 
further  that  the  largest  afferent  fillers  known  in  the 
peripheral  nerves  do  not  occur  at  all  in  the  cutaneous 
branches,  and  it  seems  clear  that  these  fibers  are  con- 
nected to  the  muscle  stretch  receptors.  To  this  extent 
then  von  Frey's  concepts  appear  valid.  The  fact  that 
tactile  stimuli  can  activate  A  fibers  of  different  sizes 
may  or  may  not  he  compatible  with  the  classic  ideas. 
What  seems  difticult  to  reconcile  with  von  Frey's  con- 
cepts is  the  suggestive  evidence  that  C  fibers  (which 
presumably  ramify  in  free  endings  only)  are  also  acti- 
vated by  tactile  stimuli.  If  this  should  be  so  a  major 
question  to  be  answered  would  be  whether  individual 
somatic  C  fibers  are  modality  specific  or  whether  an 
individual  fiber  is  e.xcited   by  tactile  as  well   as  by 


thermal  and  nociceptixe  stimuli.  If  the  latter  should 
be  the  case,  the  classical  concepts  would  clearly 
need  a  major  revision  obviouslv  in  the  direction  of  the 
ideas  expressed  by  Head. 


CENTR.\L  TACTILE  AND  KINESTHETIC  SYSTEMS 

General  Remarks 

It  is  well  known  that  the  dorsal  root  fibers  ramify 
upon  entry  into  the  central  nervous  system  and,  by 
means  of  their  main  ijranches  and  collaterals,  estab- 
lish synaptic  contacts  with  .several  nuclear  regions.  It 
is  convenient  to  divide  into  two  classes  those  regions 
to  which  discharges  aroused  by  tactile  stimuli  can  be 
relayed.  The  first  is  formed  by  regions  which  are,  or 
which  can  be  reasonably  assumed  to  be,  instrumental 
for  generation  of  tactile  sensations.  To  the  second  class 
belong  those  regions  which  are  either  not  at  all  .sen- 
sory, as  in  the  case  of  the  anterior  horn  cells,  or  those 
which  receive  afferent  information  but  for  which 
there  is  no  reason  to  believe  that  their  function  has  an 
essential  bearing  upon  tactile  or  kinesthetic  experi- 
ence. It  is  thus  clear  enough  that  the  appearance  of 
evoked  neural  activity  in  a  given  synaptic  region  fol- 
lowing tactile  stimulation  may  indeed  mean  that  the 
region  in  question  is  relevant  for  tactile  sensations. 
.Such  responses,  however,  may  equally  well  merely 
indicate  that  some  other  activ'ity,  not  necessarily  even 
sensory  in  nature,  is  modulated  by  the  activity  of 
tactile  receptors.  Considerable  confusion  exists  in  the 
literature  in  respect  to  this  problem,  since  many 
workers  seem  to  believe  that  a  response  evoked  any- 
where in  the  central  nervous  system  by  tactile  stimuli 
is  prima  facie  evidence  that  the  locus  in  question  is 
linked  somehow  to  tactile  sensations.  If  one  considers 
that  most  morphological  groupings  in  the  central 
nervous  system  establish  synaptic  contacts  with 
more  than  one  other  morphological  entity,  the 
numijer  of  potentially  activated  synaptic  regions  may 
be  expected  to  increase  in  geometrical  progression 
with  each  synaptic  relay.  It  is  likely,  therefore,  that 
within  a  short  time  a  signal  in  an  afferent  filler  could 
be  relayed,  at  least  in  principle,  to  almost  any  group- 
ing within  the  central  nervous  system. 

Hence  it  is  not  unduly  surprising  if  under  certain 
experimental  conditions  a  response  to  a  tactile  stimu- 
lus occurs  in  a  region  which  anatomically  appears  to 
be  an  altogether  unlikely  locus.  It  is  fortunate  indeed 
for  an  analysis  by  electrophysiological  methods  that 
all  potentialities  for  synaptic  transfer  are  for  a  num- 


396 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH'iSIOLOOV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


ber  of  reasons  actually  not  realized,  and  that  tactile 
and  kinesthetic  stimuli  activate  usually  only  a  limited 
number  of  synaptic  regions,  even  though  this  number 
varies  considerably  under  difTerent  experimental  con- 
ditions. 

In  the  sections  which  follow  we  shall  concern  our- 
selves almost  exclusively  with  pathways  and  synaptic 
regions  \vhich  are  demonstrably  significant  for  tactile 
and  kinesthetic  sensations.  We  shall  not  consider  the 
problem  of  how  stimulation  of  tactile  and  kinesthetic 
receptors  may  affect  other  activity  in  the  central 
nervous  system. 

Classification  oj  Central  Tactile  and  fiiiieslhetic  Systems 

It  is  well  known  that  tactile  or  kinesthetic  dis- 
charges or  both  are  conducted  centripetally  in  the 
posterior  columns  of  the  spinal  cord,  in  its  antero- 
lateral columns  and  in  the  trigeminal  pathways.  It 
also  seems  evident  that  such  impulses  can  enter  the 
central  nervous  system  through  the  roots  of  the  ninth 
and  tenth  cranial  nerves,  and  there  is  good  evidence 
(197)  that  some  chorda  tympani  fibers  may  be  acti- 
vated by  mechanoreceptors. 

It  is  both  convenient  and  almost  certainly  correct 
to  consider  together  the  systems  arising  in  the  posterior 
column  nuclei  and  the  one  arising  in  the  maiia  sensory 
trigeminal  nucleus.  We  shall  refer  to  them  as  com- 
ponents of  the  inedial  lemniscal  system.  Likewise,  we 
shall  consider  the  spinothalamic  system  as  consisting 
of  two  components.  The  first  is  the  spinothalamic 
tract  arising  in  the  posterior  horns  of  the  spinal  cord 
and  the  second  is  the  spinothalamic  tract  arising  in 
the  spinal  nucleus  of  the  fifth  nerve.  W'e  shall  refer  to 
the  latter,  following  White  &  .Sweet  (273),  as  the 
bulbothalamic  tract. 


MEDI.'^L    LEMNISCAL    SYSTEM 

Anatamical  Definition 

This  system  is  the  better  known  of  the  two,  both 
anatomically  and  functionally.  Anatomically  we  shall 
mention  here  only  the  centripetal  terminations  of  the 
successive  axons  in  the  system.  Some  collateral  con- 
nections relevant  for  our  considerations  will  be  dis- 
cussed later.  The  spinal  component  of  the  system  is 
formed  by  axons  emanating  from  the  cells  of  the 
spinal  ganglia  and  ascending  on  the  homolateral  side 
of  the  cord  in  the  posterior  column  and  synapsing  on 
cells  in  GoU  and  Burdach's  nuclei;  by  axons  originat- 


ing from  the  cells  of  these  nuclei,  crossing  (as  far  as  is 
known,  entirely)  to  the  opposite  side  and  ascending  in 
the  medial  lemniscus  to  end  upon  the  cells  of  the 
external  component  of  the  thalamic  ventrobasal 
complex;  and  by  axons  originating  in  the  cells  of  the 
latter  element  and  projecting  upon  the  postcentral 
cortex  or  its  homologue.' 

The  trigeminal  component  of  the  lemniscal  system 
arises  in  the  main  sensory  nucleus  of  the  fifth  nerve. 
There  is  complete  agreement  among  most  observers 
that  the  main  outflow  of  this  nucleus  consists  of  axons 
crossing  to  the  opposite  side.  The  pathway  adjoins 
mediodorsally  the  medial  lemniscus,  forms  an  integral 
part  of  it  and  terminates  in  the  arcuate  component  of 
the  ventrobasal  complex.  The  cells  of  the  latter 
project,  as  do  the  cells  of  the  external  element,  upon 
the  postcentral  cortex. 

In  addition  to  the  crossed  \entral  pathways  men- 
tioned aijo\e,  a  dorsal  pathway  originating  in  the 
main  sensory  nucleus  and  reaching  the  thalamus  via 
a  tegmental  route  is  frequently  described.  Consider- 
able uncertainty  prevails,  however,  about  the  origin 
of  this  tract,  its  components  and  its  terminations  in 
the  thalamus.  Wallenberg  (261)  described  an  un- 
crossed and  a  crossed  component  and  believed  that 

'  It  is  customary  to  consider  n.  vcntialis  posteromedialis  and 
n.  ventralis  posterolateralis  as  the  two  tactile  thalamic  nuclei. 
We  refer,  however,  to  the  principal  tactile  thalamic  region  as 
the  ventrobasal  complex  and  distinguish  within  this  complex  the 
arcuate  or  medial  component,  which  receives  the  trigeminal 
projection,  and  the  external  or  lateral  component,  which 
recei\es  projections  from  the  rest  of  the  body  (207).  The  reasons 
for  this  nomenclature  are  as  follows.  First,  the  two  components 
of  the  ventrobasal  complex  are  almost  identical  in  their  archi- 
tecture and  for  that  reason  should  not  be  divided  into  two 
separate  nuclei.  Actually  in  the  rabbit  such  a  separation  is  very 
difficult  while  in  the  cat  and  monkey  it  is  best  done  on  the  basis 
of  a  dividing  fibrous  lamella.  Second,  most  workers  include 
into  their  n.  ventralis  posteromedialis  a  ventromedial  element 
which  is  not  activated  by  tactile  stimuli  and  which  displays 
structural  characteristics  of  its  own,  which  are  different  from 
those  of  the  arcuate  portion  of  the  ventrobasal  complex.  Only 
Jimenez-Castellanos  (136)  and  Jasper  &  Ajmone-Marsan  (135) 
do  not  include  this  element  in  the  n.  ventralis  posteromedialis. 
The  latter  workers,  however,  consider  as  a  part  of  this  nucleus  a 
portion  of  the  posterior  thalamic  group.  Likewise,  n.  ventralis 
posterolateralis  is  with  many  workers  not  coextensive  with  the 
lateral  component  of  the  \'entrobasal  complex.  Thus  Olszewski 
(i8g),  for  example,  distinguishes  within  his  n.  ventralis  posterior 
lateralis  an  oral  part  which  is  not  activated  by  tactile  stimuli 
and  a  caudal  part  which  corresponds  probably  exactly  to  the 
lateral  component  of  the  ventrobasal  complex. 

In  respect  to  the  sensory  somatic  cortical  field  we  shall  use 
interchangeably  the  following  terms:  first  somatic  field,  post- 
central cortex  (areas  i  to  3  in  primates),  postcentral  homologue 
and  primary  receiving  area. 


most  but  not  all  of  the  fibers  of  these  tracts  terminated 
before  reaching;  the  thalamus.  Walker  (254)  confirmed 
in  essence  Wallenberg's  observations  and  concluded 
that  the  uncrossed  fibers  predominate  and  that  many 
terminate  in  the  most  medial  portion  of  the  arcuate 
nucleus.  Other  workers  (40,  117,  igi,  192)  reported 
that  the  tract  is  uncrossed  and  that  it  terminates  in 
the  arcuate  nucleus  or  in  the  centrum  medianum,  or 
in  both.  Recently  Torvik  (239)  presented  evidence  on 
the  basis  of  retrograde  cell  degeneration  that  the 
dorsomedial  sector  of  the  main  sensory  nucleus  pro- 
jects to  the  homolateral  thalamus  while  the  rest  of  this 
nucleus  projects  to  the  contralateral  side,  thus  con- 
confirming  some  older  observations  (140)  in  this 
respect. 

Since  physiological  evidence  is  conclusive  that 
both  the  contralateral  and  ipsilateral  face  areas  are 
represented  in  the  arcuate  sector  of  each  ventrobasal 
complex,  it  is  tempting  to  assume  that  the  uncrossed 
tegmental  trigeminal  pathway  exists  and  that  it  relays 
tactile  and  kinesthetic  impulses  from  the  homolateral 
face.  Moreo\er,  Hatschek's  observation  (117)  that 
the  uncrossed  tract  is  particularly  prominent  in  un- 
gulates would  fit  with  the  findings  of  Woolsey  & 
Fairman  (277)  that  the  ipsilateral  cortical  face  areas 
are  unusually  large  in  the  pig  and  sheep.  However, 
there  are  also  some  reasons  to  doubt  the  correctness 
of  this  assumption.  First,  it  is  obvious  from  the  contro- 
versy over  whether  this  tract  is  both  crossed  and  un- 
crossed, only  uncro.ssed,  or  whether  it  exists  at  all 
(214),  that  different  workers  placed  significantly 
different  lesions  in  their  animals  and  there  is  no  con- 
vincing evidence  that  this  tract  necessarilv  arises  in 
the  main  sensory  nucleus.  Second,  it  would  be  diffi- 
cult to  understand  why  homolateral  tactile  and 
kinesthetic  impulses  should  utilize  a  tract  which  is 
quite  different  in  its  fiber  composition  from  the  ven- 
tral pathway  and  why  the  homolateral  tract  should 
lie  so  far  apart  from  the  contralateral  one.  Finally,  the 
usual  observation  of  recent  workers  that  the  tract 
ends  in  the  most  medial  sector  of  the  arcuate  nucleus 
does  not  immediately  establish  that  it  relays  tactile 
impulses,  for  in  contrast  to  our  own  definition  of  the 
arcuate  component  most  workers  include  in  it  not 
only  the  tactile  thalamic  trigeminal  region  but  also 
a  ventromedial  element  (diff"ering  considcraijly  in 
structure  from  the  arcuate  nucleus)  which  in  our 
opinion  is  not  activated  by  tactile  stimuli.  It  has  been 
suggested  (207)  that  this  element  may  be  connected 
with  taste.  It  is  of  interest  to  point  out  that  von 
Economo  (243)  suggested  a  long  time  ago  that  the 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS  397 

dorsal    trigeminal    tract    is    in    fact   concerned    with 
gustatory  impulses. 

Physiological  Properties 

It  has  been  known  for  a  long  time  that  destruction 
of  the  posterior  columns  in  man  leads  to  a  loss  of  the 
capacity  to  appreciate  the  position  and  the  movement 
of  the  limbs,  and  to  an  inability  to  recognize  the  vi- 
brations of  a  tuning  fork  applied  over  the  bone.  The 
disturbances  in  tactile  sensations  were  the  subject  of 
some  dispute.  It  seems  reasonable  to  believe,  how- 
ever, that  there  is  a  severe  impairment  in  the  appre- 
ciation of  the  spatial  and  temporal  sequence  of  a 
series  of  stimuli.  In  addition,  increases  in  threshold 
for  tactile  stimuli,  a  diminution  in  the  number  of 
'sensory  spots'  and  an  impairment  in  proper  localiza- 
tion of  the  stimulus  is  often  described. 

A  general  property  of  the  lemniscal  system  is  that 
the  information  concerning  the  form,  natiu'e,  location 
and  temporal  sequences  of  the  impinging  stimuli  is 
transmitted  at  each  synaptic  station  with  great  secur- 
ity. From  the  point  of  view  of  its  organization,  the 
medial  lemniscal  system  displays  two  striking  features. 
The  first  of  these  is  that  the  peripheral  sensory  sheet 
is  projected  centrally  in  a  precise  pattern,  which  is 
preserved  to  a  considerable  degree  through  the  suc- 
cessive relays  of  the  system,  and  is  finally  impressed 
upon  the  postcentral  cortex.  The  second  is  that  the 
system  encompasses  within  a  single  tcjpographical 
pattern  several  submodalities  of  the  general  sense 
of  mechanoreception.  We  wish  to  discuss  the  system 
from  these  two  \iewpoints. 


PROJECTION   P.iiTTERNS  IN  MEDL-^L  LEMNISCAL  SYSTEM 

Patterns  in  Dorsal  Columns 

The  weight  of  the  evidence  indicates  that  the  large 
majority  of  ner\e  fibers  reaching  the  dorsal  column 
nuclei  by  way  of  the  dorsal  columns  are  axons  of 
first  order  neurons.  It  is  not  known  that  they  are 
exclusively  so,  however,  and  it  is  possible  that  some 
unknown  number  arises  from  cells  within  the  spinal 
cord,  cells  which  are  activated  by  dorsal  root  afferents 
and  are  therefore  fibers  of  the  second  order.  In  the 
cat  some  25  per  cent  of  the  dorsal  root  myelinated 
fibers  which  enter  the  dorsal  columns  at  the  seg- 
mental level  are  believed  to  reach  the  cells  of  the 
dorsal  column  nuclei  directly  (98). 

Examination  of  Marchi  degenerations  in  the  dorsal 


398 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HYSIOLOG"!' 


XEl'RnpHVSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  2.  Topical  organization  of  libers  in  the  posterior  column 
and  in  the  posterior  column  nuclei.  The  upper  two  cross  sec- 
tions (M)  refer  to  the  medulla,  the  lower  five  to  the  coccygeal 
(I.  Co.),  sacral  (I.  S.),  lumbar  (I.  L.),  thoracic  (II.  Th.)  and 
cervical  (I.  C.)  levels  of  the  spinal  cord.  The  relative  positions 
of  fibers  are  indicated  by  dots  for  the  coccygeal  fibers  and  by 
crosses,  dashes,  dots  and  dashes  and  triangles  for  fibers  of 
successively  higher  segments,  i  :  nucleus  gracilis;  2  to  4:  com- 
plex of  nucleus  cuneatus;  7 :  descending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve. 
[From  Glees  et  al.  (97).] 


columns  following  section  of  dorsal  roots,  or  transec- 
tion of  the  dorsal  columns  al  various  levels,  indicates 
that  the  centrally  projecting  fibers  are  arranged  in  an 
orderly  lamination  (43,  65,  66,  70,  97,  258).  Those 
from  each  successively  higher  segment  are  arranged 
in  a  series  of  successively  more  lateral  laminae  of  fibers 
(fig-  2). 

Patterns  in  Dorsal  Column  Nuclei 

As  figure  2  shows,  this  precise  lamellar  arrangement 
of  the  fibers  of  the  dorsal  cohunns  is  unchanged  in  the 
dorsal  column  nuclei.  Fibers  from  the  caudal  seg- 
ments terminate  in  the  most  medial  portion  of  nucleus 


gracilis,  those  from  sacral,  lumbar  and  at  least  the 
lower  six  thoracic  roots  terminate  in  successively 
more  lateralis'  placed  dorsoventrally  directed  lamel- 
lae. Glees  et  al.  Qqj')  belie\^e  that  all  thoracic  roots 
with  the  exception  of  the  first  terminate  in  this 
nucleus.  Fibers  from  the  upper  thoracic  and  from  the 
cervical  roots  terminate  in  nucleus  cuneatus  in  a 
similar  lamellar  arrangement.  Other  fibers  of  the 
upper  thoracic  and  of  the  cervical  roots  ascend  in  the 
dorsal  columns  and  terminate  in  a  topographically 
arranged  pattern  in  the  lateral  cuneate  nucleus, 
whose  cells  in  turn  project  upon  the  cerebellar  cortex. 

The  Marchi  material  suggests  the  existence  of  a 
considerable  overlap  between  the  terminals  of  neigh- 
boring fibers.  However,  it  has  been  shown  (97,  98)  by 
using  silver  staining  methods  that  intersegmental 
overlap  is  minimal,  though  intrasegmental  overlap 
of  the  fields  of  termination  occurs.  This  latter  is 
accentuated  by  the  numerous  branching  dendrites 
which  reach  into  the  synaptic  fields  of  neighboring 
cells.  The  linage  of  the  body  form  thus  composed  by 
this  projection  is  distorted  to  allow  greater  volume 
representation  for  those  body  parts  which  are  heavily 
innervated  by  afferent  fibers. 

One  looks  to  electrophysiological  methods  for  finer 
details  of  the  representation  pattern.  The  lamination 
pattern  in  the  dorsal  columns  has  been  confirmed 
(280).  It  appears,  however,  that  only  one  study  has 
been  made  of  the  projection  pattern  in  the  dorsal 
column  nuclei,  and  that  has  been  reported  in  only  a 
short  note.  Using  physiological  stimuli  Kuhn  (145) 
has  mapped  the  projection  of  the  body  surface  upon 
the  dorsal  column  nuclei.  He  found  the  ipsilateral 
body  surface  of  the  cat  to  be  represented  within  the 
caudal  portions  of  the  dorsal  column  nuclei  as  an  in- 
verted figure  of  the  animal,  with  the  tail  pointed 
dor.socaudally,  extremities  dorsally.  No  responses  were 
recorded  following  stimulation  of  the  contralateral 
side. 

Unfortunately  there  are  no  experimental  data  to 
indicate  the  pattern  of  projection  of  the  first  order 
neurons  of  the  trigeminal  nerve  upon  the  cells  of  the 
main  sensory  nucleus  of  the  fifth.  That  a  detailed  and 
well  differentiated  pattern  must  exist  therein  is  indi- 
cated by  the  pattern  formed  by  the  terminals  of  the 
second  order  elements  within  the  thalamic  relay 
nucleus  (see  fig.  3).  This  latter  pattern  contains  also 
an  ipsilateral  projection  of  the  peri-  and  intraoral 
structures  which  are  partially  superimposed  upon  the 
contralateral   pattern  of  representation  of  the  same 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS  399 


r<=i  ci 


m      ^    m    i^    --  % 
-S-    #■  '#■  •%  < 

-Q  -y^  #  c 


h.!- 


^-^ 


c=i* 


(* 


6 


■7= 


8 


FIG.  3.  Figurine  map  depicting  the  representation  of  the  body  surface  in  the  ventrobasal  thalamic 
complex  of  the  monkey,  Macacus  rhesus,  constructed  from  data  obtained  in  an  evoked  potential  experi- 
ment under  deep  barbiturate  anesthesia.  Inset  drawing  shows  diagrammatically  the  thalamic  struc- 
tures in  a  Horsely-Clarke  plane  ^frontal  plane  6).  Dots  indicate  points  at  which  electrical  activity 
was  evoked  by  tactile  stimulation  of  the  body  surface.  For  each  dot  in  the  inset  an  appropriately 
located  figurine  is  shown.  No  responses  were  obtained  elsewhere  along  the  explored  electrode  tracks. 
Body  areas,  stimulation  of  which  evoked  large,  smaller  or  small  responses  are  shown  in  the  figurines 
by  solid  black,  cross-hatching  or  diagonal  lines,  respectively.  The  body  is  represented  contralaterally 
except  for  the  face  and  intraoral  structures  which  are  bilaterally  represented.  Numerals  indicate  the 
mediolateral  and  vertical  Horsley-Clarke  coordinates.  MD:  raediodorsal  nucleus;  CM:  centrum 
medianum;  GLD:  dorsal  lateral  geniculate  body;  VBarc:  arcuate  component  of  the  ventrobasal 
complex;  VBex:  external  component  of  the  ventrobasal  complex;  VM:  ventromedial  nucleus;  /.-  the 
inferior  portion  of  the  ventral  nuclear  group.  [From  Mountcastle  &  Henneman  (185).] 


facial  regions.  Whether  this  projection  depends  upon 
ipsilaterai  axons  from  the  main  sensory  nucleus 
traveling  in  the  dorsal  trigeminal  tract  is  conjectural 
(see  p.  398). 

Patterns  in  Thalamic  Relay  Nucleus 

DEFINITION  OF  TH.^L.^Mic  RELAY  NUCLEUS.  Evidence 
from  several  experimental  approaches  indicates  that 


the  ventrobasal  complex,  consisting  of  an  external 
and  an  arcuate  portion,  is  the  thalamic  relay  for  the 
medial  lemniscal  system.  Tactile  and  kinesthetic  ac- , 
tivity  is  relayed  through  it  to  the  first  somatic  area  of 
the  cortex.  In  carnivores  and  priinates  this  region  of 
the  thalamus  is  distinguished  from  its  neighbors  in 
the  ventral  thalamic  group  by  a  special  cytoarchi- 
tecture.  It  contains  neurons  which  vary  widely  in  size. 
These  sizes  are  grouped  around  two  means,  though  in 


400 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  posterior  third  of  the  complex  this  difference  is 
less  obvious  (50).  It  is  the  thalamic  area  as  defined 
here  which  receives  the  terminals  of  axons  of  the  as- 
cending lemniscal  system  (40,  47,  48,  56,  67,  1 70, 
200,  203,  242,  252,  260),  and  it  is  this  thalamic  com- 
plex alone  which  undergoes  retrograde  degeneration 
following  lesions  of  the  cortex  confined  to  the  post- 
central homologue  (49,  50,  143,  253).  The  method 
of  local  strychninization  and  observation  of  the  in- 
duced behavioral  changes  yielded  results  in  accord 
with  these  facts,  though  the  method  is  too  crude 
for  any  detailed  analysis  (59,  60).  Finally,  electro- 
physiological experiments  are  consistent  with  the 
notion  that  the  principal  area  of  the  thalamus  acti- 
vated by  tactile  and  kinesthetic  stimulation  of  the 
body  is  coextensive  with  the  ventrobasal  complex 
(164,  182,  184,  185,  207).  Two  questions  in  this  regard 
require  further  comment. 

DIRECT  SPINOCORTICAL  AND  BULBOCORTICAL  PATH- 
WAYS. It  is  an  old  suggestion  that  certain  ascending 
sensory  somatic  fibers  of  spinal  or  dorsal  column  nuclei 
origin  might  reach  the  cerebral  cortex  directly  without 
an  intervening  synaptic  relay  in  the  diencephalon.  In 
1890,  Flechsig  &  Hosel  (69)  put  forward  this  conten- 
tion, having  found  some  degenerations  in  the  medial 
lemniscus  of  a  patient  who  died  following  a  lesion  be- 
lieved to  be  limited  to  the  cerebral  cortex.  This  idea 
was  supported  by  Tschermak  (240)  but  vigorously 
opposed  by  other  workers  who  failed  to  confirm 
Flechsig  &  Hosel's  observation  and  who  concluded 
that  the  fibers  of  the  medial  lemniscus  all  terminate 
in  the  diencephalon.  This  latter  view  is  widely  sup- 
ported by  virtually  all  more  extensive  neuroanatom- 
ical  studies  and  prevails  even  though  some  dissent- 
ing observations  are  occasionally  described  (186). 

Recently  Brodal  &  Walberg  (35)  and  Brodal  & 
Kaada  (33)  revived  again  the  question  of  the  exist- 
ence of  both  the  direct  bulbocortical  and  the  spino- 
cortical  tracts.  The  first  is  stated  to  arise  from  cells  of 
the  dorsal  column  nuclei  and  to  project  bilaterally 
upon  the  cerebral  cortex  by  a  pathway  which  joins 
the  pyramidal  tracts  of  either  side.  The  second  is  be- 
lieved to  derive  from  neurons  of  the  spinal  cord  and 
to  ascend  directly  therefrom  to  the  cortex  in  the  pyram- 
idal tracts.  Both  pathways  are  said  to  be  activated 
by  electrical  stimulation  of  either  cutaneous  or  muscle 
nerves.  However,  the  anatomical  evidence  adduced 
by  Brodal  &  Walberg  does  not  appear  to  be  suffi- 
ciently crucial  to  settle  this  old  dispute,  and  the 
electrophysiological  observations  of  Brodal  &  Kaada 


need  not  imply  the  existence  of  such  direct  pathways 
according  to  the  findings  of  Patton  &  Amassian 
(193)  and  of  Landau  (148). 

IPSILATERAL    PATHWAY    FROM    DORSAL   COLUMN    NUCLEI 

TO  VENTROBASAL  COMPLEX.  It  is  clear  from  a  large 
number  of  studies  that,  in  so  far  as  anatomical 
methods  can  determine,  the  entire  upward  outflow  of 
the  dorsal  column  nuclei  ascends  to  the  thalamus  of 
the  contralateral  side  and  terminates  largely  within 
the  ventrobasal  thalamic  coinplex.  These  observations 
accord  well  with  the  results  of  electrophysiological 
mapping  experiments,  which  indicate  that  only  the 
contralateral  body  surface  is  projected  via  the  lem- 
niscal system  upon  the  ventrobasal  complex,  while 
the  trigeminal  component  of  this  system  does  contain 
an  ipsilateral  component,  partially  overlaid  with  the 
contralateral  one.  This  pattern  of  projection  is  fur- 
ther confirmed  by  our  single  unit  observations  in  the 
thalamus  (Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpub- 
lished observations).  Moreover,  single  unit  studies  of 
the  postcentral  homologue  in  cats  and  monkeys 
indicate  that  its  cells  are  activated  only  by  stimulation 
of  the  contralateral  body  surface,  except  for  the  tri- 
geminal inflow  (181;  and  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  & 
T.  P.  .S.  Powell,  manuscript  in  preparation).  Many 
observers  do  not  agree  with  these  findings,  however, 
and  they  report  ipsilateral  responses  in  the  region  of 
the  thalamus,  evoked  by  natural  stimuli  or  by  periph- 
eral nerve,  brachial  plexus  or  dorsal  column  electrical 
stimulation  (20,  51,  52,  90,  91,  116)  although  Berry 
et  al.  (20)  found  that  direct  electrical  stimulation  of 
one  dorsal  column  evokes  electrical  activity  only  in 
the  contralateral  thalamus.  The  latter  observation  is 
of  interest  for  it  may  provide  a  clue  for  the  interpre- 
tation of  the  divergent  findings.  E\idence  is  accumu- 
lating (see  p.  419)  that  in  contrast  to  the  medial 
lemniscal  system  the  spinothalamic  system  does  pos- 
sess an  ipsilateral  component  from  the  body  surface, 
which  may  terminate  partlv  or  wholly  in  the  .segment 
of  the  posterior  thalamic  group  which  adjoins  the 
ventrobasal  coinplex  posteriorly.  It  seems  possible 
that  the  workers  who  obtained  ipsilateral  responses 
from  stimulation  of  the  body  surface  or  nerves  ob- 
tained them  actually  in  the  region  which  lies  pos- 
teriorly to  the  ventrobasal  complex.  While  this  inter- 
pretation would  harmonize  the  existing  discordant 
findings,  it  would  not  immediately  explain  why  ip- 
silateral stimuli  fail  to  actix'ate  (at  least  under  condi- 
tions of  moderate  anesthesia)  the  ventrobasal  complex 
itself — as  could  be  expected — unless  one  assumes  that 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


401 


the  functional  significance  of  the  ipsilateral  inflow 
diflPers  materially  from  that  of  the  contralateral  one. 

PATTERNS  IN  TACTILE  THALAMIC  AREA.  Detailed  in- 
formation regarding  the  pattern  of  projection  of  the 
lemniscal  system  upon  its  thalamic  rela\'  nucleus  is 
provided  by  studies  using  the  e\oked  potential  tech- 
nique. This  method,  as  applied  to  study  of  the  thala- 
mus, involves  passing  a  recording  electrode  down 
through  the  thalamus  in  successive  rows  of  penetra- 
tions so  placed  as  to  explore  the  thalamic  areas  acti- 
vated. At  successive  intervals  during  its  downward 
passage  the  electrode  is  held  stationary  and  the  area 
of  the  body  surface  in  which  stimulation  evokes  elec- 
trical activitv  at  a  given  point  is  determined.  The 
figurine  drawings  which  can  be  constructed  from  the 
data  for  each  point  are  placed  in  proper  relation  to 
one  another  and  to  the  thalamic  nuclear  outlines,  as 
determined  by  study  of  the  serial  sections  of  the  ex- 
perimental brains  (182,  184,  185,  207).  The  pattern 
of  representation  in  the  monkey  is  shown  in  figure  3. 

Analysis  of  this  figure  reveals  that  the  body  surface 
of  the  monkey  is  represented  as  a  distorted  image  of 
the  animal.  The  face  and  head  are  represented  within 
the  arcuate  portion  of  the  ventrobasal  complex,  the 
liody  in  its  external  element.  The  middorsal  line  of  the 
body  from  nose  to  tail  is  represented  across  the  top 
of  the  complex,  the  trunk  and  girdle  regions,  proxi- 
mal and  then  distal  parts  of  the  extremities  in  suc- 
cessively more  ventral  positions.  The  only  ipsilateral 
projection  is  that  of  the  peri-  and  intraoral  regions. 

Perusal  of  such  a  figurine  map  makes  it  clear  that 
a  given  small  area  of  the  body  surface  is  not  repre- 
sented at  a  thalamic  '  point'  and  only  there.  Stimula- 
tion of  a  small  spot  on  the  skin  evokes  intense  activity 
at  a  limited  thalamic  locus  and  less  intense  activity 
over  a  considerable  surround.  It  follows  that  a  given 


thalamic  locus  can  be  activated  to  some  degree  from 
a  considerable  area  of  skin,  which  is  smaller  for  some 
and  larger  for  other  parts  of  the  topographical  pat- 
tern. As  the  peripheral  spot  stimulated  is  shifted  across 
the  skin  the  peak  activity  shifts  across  the  thalamic 
pattern,  its  submaximal  and  liminal  fringes  shifting 
with  it.  The  problem  is  to  understand  the  precision 
of  spatial  discrimination  of  which  the  organism  is 
capable,  which  depends  upon  an  anatomical  sub- 
strate of  '  point  to  area'  and  reciprocallv,  'area  to 
point'  projection  of  the  receptor  sheet  upon  the  central 
configurations.  Some  physiological  mechanisms  which 
appear  of  importance  in  this  regard  will  be  considered 
later. 

When  the  representation  pattern  shown  in  cross- 
section  in  figure  3  is  analyzed  in  three  dimensions,  it 
results  that  any  given  dermatomal  (segmental)  region 
of  the  body  is  represented  in  the  ventrobasal  complex 
in  a  narrow  curving  lamella  of  tissue,  concave  medi- 
all\".  Within  such  a  narrow  sheet  the  proximal  skin 
areas  of  the  dermatome  are  represented  dorsallv,  the 
distal  ventrally. 

Extension  of  such  studies  to  a  series  of  mammals 
allows  some  estimate  of  the  phyletic  trends  in  thalamic 
tactile  representation.  The  sequence  of  that  repre- 
sentation is  in  principle  the  same  in  the  rabi)it,  cat 
and  monkey  (fig.  4).  The  entire  body  surface  is  repre- 
.sented  in  each  case,  but  striking  differences  in  em- 
phasis exist.  In  the  rabbit,  the  bulk  of  the  available 
tissue  is  given  to  the  projection  of  head  and  face, 
while  the  cat  possesses  a  ijalanced  spinal  and  tri- 
geminal projection.  In  the  monkey  the  increased  de- 
velopment of  the  hand  and  foot  as  tactile  organs  is 
indicated  by  an  increased  share  of  the  pattern  de- 
voted to  their  representation. 

This  general  pattern  depicted  by  electrophysio- 
logical studies  is  a  confirmation  and  extension  of  that 


FIG.  4.  Schematic  outlines  of  body  representation  in  the  ventrobasal  thalamic  complex  in  rabbit, 
cat  and  monkey.  The  figures  do  not  intend  to  depict  with  accuracy  the  actual  relationships  but  aim 
to  emphasize  the  dominance  of  the  trigeminal  representation  in  the  rabbit,  and  the  relative  increase 
of  the  representation  of  the  limbs  in  cat  and  monkey.  The  representation  of  the  ti'unk  and  extremities 
is  located  quite  anteriorly  in  the  ventrobasal  complex  of  the  rabbit.  In  the  cat  and  monkey  this 
representation  reaches  progi'essively  very  much  farther  caudally. 


402 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


demonstrated  anatomically.  The  termination  of 
gracilis  neurons  in  the  lateral  and  of  those  of  the 
cuneate  in  the  medial  parts  of  the  external  component 
of  the  ventrobasal  complex,  and  of  the  trigeminal 
tracts  in  its  arcuate  component,  has  been  established 
by  degeneration  experiments  (47,  67,  203,  253,  260). 
The  same  pattern  is  shown  by  the  locations  of  retro- 
grade degenerations  produced  by  lesions  of  the  face, 
arm  or  leg  areas  of  the  postcentral  gyrus  (49,  50,  143, 

249.  253)- 

An  important  confirmation  of  the  location  of  the 

somatic  relay  nucleus  of  the  thalamus  and  the  pattern 
in  it  has  come  from  the  study  of  patients  in  whom  the 
ventral  thalamic  nuclei  were  stimulated  by  means  of 
stereotactically  placed  electrodes  in  the  course  of 
thalamotomy  for  intractable  pain  (174,  175)-  Stimu- 
lation of  the  ventrobasal  complex  produced  somatic 
sensations  referred  to  bodily  parts  in  topographic 
patterns  similar  to  those  in  the  monkey.  The  sensa- 
tions produced  by  thalamic  stimulation  were  referred 
onl\'  to  the  contralateral  side  of  the  bodv. 


Patterns  in  Postcentral  Humologue  oj  Cerebral  Cortex 

Since  sensory  somatic  cortical  projection  patterns 
are  described  by  Terzuolo  &  Adey,  Chapter  XXXIII 
of  this  work,  it  may  he  read  for  a  surves'  of  this  in- 
formation and  the  description  of  relations  between  the 
sensory  somatic  fields  and  the  motor  areas. 

Here  we  wish  to  stress  a  few  generalizations  impor- 
tant for  our  considerations.  The  first  essential  point 
which  emerges  from  the  extensive  mapping  studies 
made  mainly  by  Woolsey  and  his  collaborators  is  that 
the  cortical  pattern  in  every  mammal  studied  is  a 
representation  of  the  body  form  itself,  with  distor- 
tions which  are  almost  certainly  due  to  differences  in 
the  peripheral  innervation  density.  These  in  turn  ap- 
pear clearly  related  to  the  development  of  one  or  an- 
other part  of  the  body  of  a  given  mainmal  as  a  tactile 
organ.  Hence,  the  share  of  a  body  part  in  cortical 
representation  apparently  reflects  the  relative  value 
of  this  part  for  tactile  discriminatory  acuity. 

The  second  point  to  be  made  is  that  the  thalamic 
pattern  is  projected  in  toto  upon  the  cortical  receiving 
area  with  only  such  further  distortions  as  could  be 
expected  by  the  transfer  of  a  three-dimensional  pat- 
tern upon  essentially  a  two-dimensional  surface. 
While  this  statement  is  made  on  the  basis  of  studies 
done  only  in  rabbit,  cat  and  monkey,  there  is  no  rea- 
son to  doubt  that  it  is  true  for  other  mammals  as  well. 
There  appears  to  be  no  part  of  the  thalamic  relay 


nucleus  which  is  functionally  independent  of  the 
cortex,  a  finding  which  is  concordant  with  the  obser- 
vation that  an  adequate  cortical  removal  results  in 
virtually  complete  retrograde  degeneration  of  the 
ventrobasal  complex.  There  is,  therefore,  no  reason  to 
assume  that  this  complex  is  a  terminal  station  for  any 
sensory  somatic  processes  in  any  mammal,  even 
though  a  contrary  thought  in  this  respect  was  fre- 
quently entertained  in  the  past,  and  is  implicit  in  the 
concept  of  'thalamic  sensations'.  For  any  given  mam- 
mal, the  pattern  of  cortical  representation  is  probably 
essentially  similar  not  only  to  the  respective  repre- 
sentation in  the  ventrobasal  complex  but  to  that  in 
the  dorsal  column  nuclei  as  well.  The  a\ailai3le  data 
in  this  regard  are  very  limited.  It  is  clear  enough, 
however,  that  this  holds  true  for  the  cat  and  the  same 
can  be  deduced  for  the  macaque.  Moreover,  the  long, 
sentient  and  prehensile  tail  of  the  spider  monkey 
Ateles  is  known  to  have  a  large  representation  in  the 
dorsal  column  nuclei  ('43)  and  this  rather  unusual  dis- 
tortion of  the  pattern  is  fully  reflected  in  the  cortical 
representation  (45).  It  appears  then  that  all  relay 
nuclei  of  the  system  participate  fully  in  elaboration 
of  the  projectional  pattern,  as  must  be  the  ca.se  if  the 
organization  of  this  projection  is  correlated  with  the 
peripheral  innervation  density.  While  this  conclusion 
appears  to  be  almost  self-evident,  it  may  be  useful  to 
stress  it  since  even  modern  neurological  thinking  is 
often  unduly  dominated  by  the  concept  of  different 
fimctional  levels.  This  tends  to  neglect  the  viewing  of 
the  synaptic  regions  of  a  system  as  integral  parts  of 
the  whole,  if  such  regions  happen  to  lie  at  different 
topographical  levels. 

An  important  question  as  to  the  functional  mean- 
ing of  a  morphological  pattern  is  posed  by  the  c\to- 
architectonic  differentiation  of  the  postcentral  hom- 
ologue.  While  the  number  of  fields  distinguished  here 
may  vary  according  to  different  criteria  of  various 
workers,  there  is  hardly  any  doubt  that  this  region 
possesses  a  definite  gradient  of  morphological  change. 
In  primates,  areas  3,  i,  and  2  are  classically  distin- 
guished in  an  orocaudal  sequence  and  all  these  fields 
together  form  the  substrate  for  the  representation  pat- 
tern of  the  body  as  determined  by  the  evoked  poten- 
tial technique.  It  is  possible  that  differences  in  or- 
ganization of  thalamocortical  projections  underlie 
the  cytoarchitectural  differentiation  of  these  fields. 
In  this  relation  it  was  reported  recently  (50)  that  at 
least  areas  3  and  2  differ  substantially  in  this  regard 
from  each  other.  Area  3  has  been  shown  to  receive 
exclusive  projections  from  the  ventrobasal  complex. 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


403 


while  area  2  seems  to  receive  only  a  collateral  outflow 
from  it.  What  this  important  finding  may  imply  lunc- 
tionally  is  at  present  oiascure. 


MODALITY    COMPONENTS   OF   MEDIAL    LEMNISCAL   SYSTEM 


is  induced  by  general  anesthesia,  however  light.  In 
any  case,  it  is  at  present  both  convenient  and  necessary 
to  consider  separately  the  activity  in  the  lemniscal 
system  evoked  by  stimulation  of  the  skin,  touch- 
pressure,  and  that  provoked  by  stimulation  of  perios- 
teum, bones  and  joints,  deep  sensibility. 


A  second  general  property  of  the  lemni-scal  system 
is  that  it  handles,  within  a  single  topographical  pat- 
tern, activity  evoked  Ijy  tactile  as  well  as  kinesthetic 
and  other  mechanical  stimuli  acting  upon  deep  tis- 
sues. At  each  successive  level  of  the  system  neurons 
subserving  various  forms  of  mechanoreception  are  in- 
termingled in  a  common  topographical  pattern. 
Nevertheless,  single  unit  studies  indicate  that  the  in- 
dividual neurons  at  each  level  retain  their  modality 
specificity.  This  rather  surprising  observation  requires 
an  immediate  comment.  In  work  with  an  intact  ani- 
mal a  given  unit  at  a  central  station  of  the  system 
can  be  driven  by  stimuli  delivered  to  the  skin  or  to  the 
deep  tissues.  It  is  usually  a  simple  matter  to  be  certain 
which  of  the  two  contains  the  effective  receptors,  and 
this  can  be  proved  by  direct  surgical  dissection  of  the 
peripheral  tissues.  Some  difficulty  does  arise  when 
the  receptive  fields  lie  in  highly  specialized  regions  at 
the  apices  of  the  limbs,  such  as  the  claws  of  the  cat. 
Nevertheless,  the  lack  of  any  evidence  that  stimuli  to 
the  skin  and  to  the  deep  tissues  can  excite  the  same 
neuron  is  quite  striking.  Since  all  the  findings  are 
derived  from  anesthetized  preparations  it  is  con- 
ceivable, although  we  believe  rather  unlikely,  that 
his  apparent  lack  of  any  clear  excitatory  interaction 


Touch-Pressure 

.\D.^PTIVE  PROPERTIES  OF  RECEPTORS  AND  OF  CENTRAL 

NEURONS.  It  has  been  known  for  a  long  time  (2,  3,  6} 
that  the  mechanoreceptors  of  the  skin  and  the  afferent 
fibers  to  which  they  are  connected  are  not  uniform  in 
all  their  properties.  One  can  classify  these  afferents 
according  to  the  following  criteria :  the  adequate 
stimulus  required  for  each,  the  size  and  conduction 
velocity  of  the  fibers  concerned,  the  rate  of  adaptation 
to  steady  stimuli  and  the  sizes  of  the  peripheral  recep- 
tive fields  (168,  280,  282).  When  working  with  the 
intact  anesthetized  animal,  however,  it  is  useful  to 
classify  the  cutaneous  mechanoreceptors  as  (i)  those 
which  respond  steadily  to  steady  stimuli  and  (it)  those 
which  adapt  quickly  to  such  stimuli  (fig.  5).  Neural 
elements  at  each  of  the  central  relay  stations  of  the 
lemniscal  system,  which  are  driven  by  mechanical 
stimulation  of  the  skin,  fall  readiK'  into  one  or  the 
other  of  these  classes  (2,  3,  6,  7,  181 ;  Berman,  A.  L., 
unpublished  observations,  and  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  & 
J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished  observations).  The  type  of 
adaptation  of  a  given  unit  is,  so  far  as  has  been  ob- 
served, an  unchanging  functional  property.  In  general, 
afferents  related  to  hairs  are  quickly  adapting,  while 


On 


Pressure 


Off 

II 


Moving  one  thread  of  hair 


J 


(        «-\ 


^A. 


\"  n 


FIG.  5.  .-Xction  potentials  in  single  cutaneous  nerve  fibers  of  the  cat,  elicited  by  mechanical  stimula- 
tion of  the  skin.  A :  \  single  fiber  adapts  rapidly  to  steady  pressure  applied  to  its  receptive  field, 
shown  in  the  inset  drawing.  A  short  train  of  impulses  occurs  at  the  onset  and  release  of  the  pressure. 
In  the  second  record  a  fiber  responds  to  movement  of  a  single  hair.  B:  The  receptive  field  for  this 
particular  fiber  is  punctiform.  The  fiber  adapts  slowly  to  a  steady  mechanical  stimulus.  Upper  row  of 
dots  apply  to  the  first  two,  the  lower  row  to  the  third  record.  Distances  between  the  dots  indicate  10 
msec,  intervals.  [From  Maruhashi  et  al.  (168).] 


404 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


slowK'  adapting  'skin  pressure'  units  arc  driven  by- 
light  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  skin  surface.  This 
correlation  is  probaljly  not  perfect,  for  some  units 
excited  by  movement  of  specialized  hairs  have  been 
observed  which  adapted  slowly  (68),  and  some  rapidly 
adapting  units  have  been  noted  whose  peripheral  re- 
ceptive fields  were  located  in  hairless  parts  of  the  skin. 
Neurons  responding  steadily  or  with  only  an  onset 
transient  to  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  skin  have 
been  observed  at  the  level  of  the  ventrobasal  thalamic 
complex  and  in  the  postcentral  homologue  (181;  and 
Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished 
obser\ations).  Records  of  the  action  potentials 
of  a  postcentral  cortical  neuron  responding  stead- 
ily to  a  steady  cutaneous  stimulus  are  shown  in 
figure  6.  Results  such  as  these  indicate  that  neurons 
located  at  the  various  levels  of  the  system  reflect  rather 
faithfully  the  discharge  properties  of  either  the  periph- 
eral receptors  themselves  or  those  of  the  first  order 
neurons.  At  each  level  of  the  svstem  the  fast  and  slowlv 


adapting  neurons  are  intermingled  within  a  single 
topographical  pattern. 

PERiPHER.AL  RECEPTIVE  FIELDS.  Quantitative  measure- 
ments of  the  cutaneous  fields  of  distribution  of  single 
fibers  have  been  very  few.  By  recording  unitary  action 
potentials  from  fibers  within  the  dorsal  columns, 
Yamamoto  et  al.  (280)  found  the  peripheral  fields  to 
vary  in  size  from  a  maximum  of  2  to  3  cm'-  on  the 
trunk,  to  a  few  square  millimeters  at  the  distal  ends 
of  the  limbs.  Working  with  single  fibers  in  cutaneous 
ner\es,  Maruhashi  et  al.  (168)  have  in  general  found 
similar  results,  though  they  emphasize  that  a)  many 
large  afferents  may  have  truly  'spot-like'  receptive 
fields,  and  6)  that  smaller  (3  to  5  n)  slowly  adapting 
afferents  may  have  wide  receptive  fields  in  the  range 
of  1 4  to  40  cm'-. 

Only  scattered  data  are  available  concerning  the 
size  of  the  fields  which  project  upon  neurons  of  the 
dorsal  column  nuclei  and  of  the  ventrobasal  complex. 


rMPULSES  PER   SECOND 


CORTICAL     NEURON    RESPONDING    TO  STEADY 
PRESSURE    OF    SKIN    OF    FOREARM 


FIG.  6.  .\ction  potentials  of  a  single  cell  in  the  postcentral  gyrus  of  the  monkey,  Macacus  rhesus.  The 
neuron  is  driven  by  steady  pressure  applied  to  the  cutaneous  receptive  field  located  on  the  volar 
surface  of  the  contralateral  forearm.  Onset  and  release  of  pressure  indicated  by  solid  bar  under  the 
graph,  which  shows  the  number  of  impulses  per  second  plotted  at  200  msec,  intervals.  Experimental 
conditions  described  by  Mountcastle  el  at.  (183).  [From  Mountcastle,  'V.  B.  &  T.  P.  .S  Powell, 
manuscript  in  preparation.] 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


405 


FIG.  7.  Eight  excitatory  peripheral  receptive  skin  fields  of 
the  cat's  foreleg,  stiinulation  of  which  activated  eight  single 
neurons  in  the  contralateral  postcentral  cortex.  The  neurons  in 
question  were  isolated  at  the  levels  indicated  (in  ii)  below  the 
cortical  surface  in  the  course  of  a  single  microelectrodc  penetra- 
tion made  perpendicularly  to  that  surface.  The  fields  are 
restricted  in  size  and  are  almost  identical  in  location.  [Modified 
from  Mountcastle  (181).] 

Some  measurements,  however,  have  been  made  of  the 
fields  which  project  upon  neurons  of  the  cerebral  cor- 
tex (181).  Several  such  fields  are  shown  in  figure  7, 
and  the  graph  of  figure  8  relates  the  sizes  of  the 
peripheral  fields  to  their  location  upon  the  body 
surface. 

PROJECTION    OF    PERIPHER.JiL    RECEPTIVE    FIELDS    UPON 

CENTRAL  NEURONS.  Figure  9  indicates  that  not  all 
parts  of  the  peripheral  receptive  field  of  a  thalamic 
neuron  have  an  equal  potency  for  excitation  of  that 
cell.  The  security  of  the  relation  varies  from  a  ma.x- 
imum  usually,  though  not  always,  near  the  center 
of  the  field  to  a  minimum  at  its  edge.  The  synaptic 
linkages  converging  upon  the  central  neuron  from 
afferent  fibers  innervating  the  edge  of  the  field  are 
apparently  .so  few  as  to  provoke  only  minimal  activa- 
tion of  the  cell,  as  measured  by  the  early  repetitive 
response  (which  will  be  defined  below). 

From  data  of  this  kind  it  is  possible  to  reconstruct 
the  pattern  of  events  set  in  motion  in  the  lemniscal 
system  by  a  brief  mechanical  stimulus  delivered  to  the 


skin.  Before  doing  so  it  is  convenient  to  describe  the 
response  properties  of  single  neurons  of  the  system. 

Response  Patterns  of  Neurons  of  Medial  Lemniscal  System 

REPETITIVENESS   OF  RESPONSE   TO  SINGLE   STIMULUS.    All 

the  evidence  at  hand  from  study  of  first  order  axons 
(168,  280)  indicates  that  even  very  brief  mechanical 
stimuli  to  hairs  or  skin  surface  elicit  a  short  train  of 
impulses  in  afferent  nerve  fibers  (see  fig.  5),  even  for 
quickly  adapting  elements.  Such  an  aff'erent  input 
elicits  from  the  second  (15),  third  (208)  and  fourth 
order  neurons  (183)  short  high  frequency  trains  of  dis- 
charges, a  response  pattern  which  is  highly  character- 
istic of  the  system  (fig.  10).  Amassian  &  DeVito  (15) 
have  shown  that  the  early  repetitive  discharge  in  the 
cuneate  nucleus  occurs  under  different  conditions  of 
anesthesia,  in  the  unanesthetized  or  decerebrate  ani- 
mal, and  apart  from  variation  of  body  temperature 
from  33  to  41  °C.  It  is  important  to  emphasize  that  it 
occurs  also  when  the  afferent  volley  is  made  up  of  a 
single  impulse  in  each  synchronously  active  fiber.  The 
repetitive  discharge  therefore  is  a  general  property  of 
the  postsynaptic  cell  at  the  first  relay  of  the  system, 
and  indeed  of  those  at  each  successive  relay  thereafter. 
The  repetitive  response  is  not  absolutely  stable  even 
in  the  deeply  anesthetized  animal.  Here,  when  exactly 
the  same  peripheral  stimulus  activating  a  given  neuron 
is  repeated  at  slow  intervals  some  variation  in  the 
number  of  impulses  in  the  early  repetitive  response 
does  indeed  occur.  In  a  population  of  such  responses, 
many  contain  a  characteristic  number  of  impulses 
per  response  (the  modal  value)  while  some  responses 
contain  more  and  others  fewer  impulses  (fig.  1 1).  The 
shift  in  the  modal  value  indicates  sensitively  the 
changing  parameters  of  the  stimulus,  e.g.  its  intensity, 
frequency  or  position  (see  figs.  9,  10,  12  and  13). 

RESPONSE  OF  SYSTEM  TO  SINGLE  STIMULUS.  Considering 
the  variations  in  the  response  of  a  single  neuron  when 
the  stimulus  shifts  across  the  receptive  field  it  seems 
possible  to  reconstruct  the  events  in  a  population  of 
cells  set  in  motion  by  a  single  stimulus,  even  though 
it  has  not  yet  been  possible  to  record  the  activity  of 
many  single  neurons  simultaneously.  .\  brief,  strong 
peripheral  stimulus  sets  up  a  burst  of  impulses  in  a 
number  of  afferent  fibers.  If  the  stimulus  is  brief 
enough  only  one  impulse  occurs  in  each  fiber;  if  it  is 
strong  enough  nearly  all  fibers  are  activated  syn- 
chronously. The  impulses  are  conducted  into  the  cord 
and  can   be  assumed   to  impinge  upon   a   restricted 


4o6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


CM' 


PERIPHERAL  RECEPTIVE  AREAS 
PROJECTING  UPON  CORTICAL  NEURONS 
OF  SOMATIC  AREA  I 
n  =  126 


THORACIC  UNITS 
27.7  ±  5.0  CM» 
n  =21 


XI0I±I3 


X95±I9 


X707ttO 


.       •X''9l05r 


X306t05 


-    J  X'I610I4 


•^1/" 


liLC 


::^ 


CM   FROM  TIP  OF  FORELIMB 

FIG.  8.  Plot  relating  the  size  (in  square  centimeters)  of  excitatory  cutaneous  receptive  fields  for 
cortical  neurons  to  the  distances  of  the  centers  of  those  fields  from  the  tip  of  the  forelimb  of  the  cat. 
Crossed  dots  locate  means  (and  standard  errors)  for  these  fields  when  .grouped  into  classes  by  4  cm 
distances  from  limb  tip.  The  receptive  fields  close  to  the  tip  of  the  limb  are  small  in  size  and  usually 
comparable  in  area.  With  the  increased  distance  from  the  tip  both  the  size  of  the  fields  and  the 
variability  between  them  increase  greatly.  [From  Mountcastle  (181).] 


group  of  cells  within  the  dorsal  column  nuclei.  The 
cells  in  the  center  of  the  group  will  receive  a  maximum 
number  of  synaptic  impingements  and  will  discharge 
repetitive  trains  at  high  frequency,  probably  with 
shortest  latency.  Cells  surrounding  the  center  are  pre- 
sumably excited  to  discharge  trains  of  fewer  impulses 
at  longer  latency  and  at  lower  frequency,  while  cells 
at  the  edges  of  the  discharge  zone  will  discharge 
single  impulses  at  even  longer  latencies.  That  a  similar 
distribution  of  activity  occurs  among  third  order  cells 
of  the  thalamic  relay  nucleus  and  among  fourth  order 
cells  in  the  cortical  receiving  area  can  be  deduced 
from  single  unit  studies  of  those  regions  (13,  157- 
■59.  183,  208). 


Marshall  et  al.  (167)  have  shown  for  the  thalamic  and 
cortical  slow  waves  that  the  recovery  time  is  greatly 
prolonged  by  anesthetic  agents.  Single  unit  studies 
have  confirmed  and  extended  these  original  observa- 
tions (13,  183;  and  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose, 
unpublished  observations).  The  recosery  time  of 
single  neurons  in  the  imanesthetized  animal  is  not  yet 
known,  though  it  probably  is  much  briefer  than  the 
recovery  time  observed  in  an  anesthetized  animal.  In 
the  anesthetized  animal,  however,  the  anesthesia 
itself  is  not  the  only  factor  affecting  the  recovery  tiine. 
At  the  same  anesthetic  level  the  unresponsive  time  of 
the  system  shortens  as  the  strength  of  the  initial 
stimulus  decreases. 


RESPONSES   TO    TWO   STIMULI    AT   DIFFERENT   INTERVALS. 

Some  information  concerning  the  capacity  of  the 
somatic  afferent  system  to  relay  activity  has  been  oij- 
tained  by  measuring  the  ability  of  the  system  to 
respond  to  a  second  peripheral  stimulus  at  various 
time    intervals   after    the    first.    Marshall   ('66)    and 


RESPONSES  TO  REPETITIVE  STIMULI  AT  DIFFERENT  FRE- 
QUENCIES. It  is  clear  from  study  of  indi\-iclual  neurons 
that  the  two-stimulus  experiment  does  not  at  all 
specifv  the  capacity  of  the  system  to  respond  when 
trains  of  stimuli  are  applied.  Individual  neurons  at 
any  level  show  one  of  two  types  of  behavior.  Figure  1 2 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


407 


^^A^ 


16    M    12     10      8        6       4       2        I        3       5       7       9      II 
LOCATION    OF    THE     STIMULATED    POINT 


13     15 


FIG.  9.  Graph  relating  the  average  number  ot  impulses 
discharged  per  response  to  the  position  of  the  stimulus  at 
several  points  located  in  and  around  the  receptive  field.  Inset 
drawing  on  the  left  indicates  the  location  of  the  receptive  field; 
inset  drawing  on  right  the  positions  of  the  stimulated  points. 
Electrical  stimulus  of  the  same  strength  is  delivered  through  a 
pair  of  needle  electrodes  thrust  into  the  skin.  The  same  unit 
located  in  the  ventrobasal  complex  of  the  thalamus  is  respond- 
ing throughout.  Graph  is  based  on  1208  responses.  Cat  under 
deep  pentobarbital  anesthesia.  [From  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  & 
J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished  observations.] 


illustrates  the  first  type,  shown  here  for  a  thalamic  cell. 
Several  characteristic  phenomena  for  this  type  of 
response  are  as  follows: 

a)  Equilibration.  The  neuron  follows  the  stiinulus 
rate  beat-for-beat  to  a  certain  level,  usually  in  the 
range  of  30  to  70  per  sec.  When  trains  of  stintuli  at 
higher  rates  are  delivered,  the  neuron  continues  to 
respond  at  about  the  same  overall  rate.  There  is  no 
desynchronization,  however,  for  each  response  occurs 
in  a  fixed  and  definite  relation  to  a  particularstimulus. 
The  equilibration  occurs  JDecause  some  stimuli  ran- 
domly distributed  throughout  the  train  fail  to  evoke 
responses. 

6)  Early  silent  period.  The  records  of  figure  1 2  show 
that  while  the  first  stimulus  elicits  a  response,  the  next 
few  (i.e.  numbers  2,  3,  4,  5,  etc.,  in  fig.  12)  may  be 
ineffectual.  The  succeeding  stimuli,  however,  once 
again  become  effective.  This  early  silent  period  is  of 
about  the  same  duration  as  the  unresponsive  time  of 


the  system,  as  measured  in  the  two  stimulus  experi- 
ment under  the  saine  experimental  conditions.  The 
important  point  is  that  the  presentation  of  trains  of 
stimuli    '  recruits    excitabilitv'    so    that    the    svstem 


FIG.  10.  Shifts  in  the  number  of  impulses  per  response  and 
changes  in  the  latent  periods  with  increased  strength  of  the 
peripheral  stimulus  for  three  units  located  at  successively  higher 
synaptic  regions  of  the  medial  lemniscal  system  of  the  cat. 
Stimulus  strength  increases  in  each  column  from  above  down- 
ward. Time  lines  for  all  columns,  1000  cycles  per  sec.  B:  Dis- 
charges of  a  single  neuron  of  the  cuneate  nucleus  evoked  by 
stimulation  of  the  ipsilateral  radial  cutaneous  nerve.  Note  the 
shift  in  latencies  and  the  increase  in  the  number  of  spikes  with 
increase  in  stimulus  strength.  The  strength  of  the  stimulus  is 
indicated  by  the  traces  at  the  extreme  left  {A}  which  show 
increases  in  the  size  of  the  compound  action  potential  in  the 
radial  nerve.  [From  Amassian  &  DeVito  (15).]  C:  Increase  in 
number  of  impulses  of  the  early  repetitive  response  of  a  single 
neuron  of  the  ventrobasal  thalamic  complex  evoked  by  in- 
creasingly stronger  electrical  stimuli  (ai)  delivered  to  the  skin  of 
the  contralateral  first  digit  of  the  forepaw.  Traces  show  the 
modal  number  of  the  discharge  train  at  each  stimulus  strength 
and  a  latency  which  is  close  to  the  mean  latency  at  this  strength. 
[From  Rose  &  Mountcastle  (208).]  D:  A  similar  series  for  a 
neuron  located  in  the  first  som.atic  cortical  field.  Electrical 
stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  contralateral  foreleg.  Each  trace 
shows  again  the  modal  number  for  the  discharge  train  at  given 
strength  of  the  stimulus  and  the  latency,  near  to  the  mean 
latency  at  this  strength.  [From  Mountcastle  el  a!.  (183).] 


4o8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


12  3  4  5 

Number   of    spikes    per   response 


FIG.  1 1 .  Curves  illustrating  the  distributions  of  response 
populations  around  their  modal  values  for  several  neurons  of 
the  thalamic  ventrobasal  nuclear  complex  of  the  cat.  Number  of 
impulses  per  response  to  a  brief  peripheral  stimulus  graphed 
against  the  percentage  of  responses  with  the  stated  number  of 
impulses.  Although  values  on  the  abscissa  are  always  integers, 
the  points  belonging  to  the  same  distribution  are  connected  by 
lines  to  aid  the  eye.  The  number  which  follows  the  letters 
TM  identifies  the  experiment,  the  number  in  parentheses 
identifies  the  neuron  studied.  .V  indicates  the  number  of 
responses  upon  which  each  graph  is  based.  Locus  of  peripheral 
stimulus  constant  for  each  unit.  In  each  case  the  neuron  is 
activated  by  electrical  stimulation  of  the  contralateral  skin. 
Locus  of  stimulation:  TM  34(1),  second  toe  on  hindfoot; 
TM  32(2),  lower  abdomen;  TM  32(5),  upper  thigh;  TM  32(6), 
ankle;  TM  27(2),  wrist;  TM  28(2),  wrist.  Note  that  most 
responses  in  each  series  do  not  differ  from  the  modal  value  by 
more  than  one  impulse.  [From  Rose  &  Mountcastle  (208).] 


transmits  at  a  higher  frequency  than  that  predicted 
by  the  recovery  cycle  studies. 

c)  Mode  reduction.  The  records  of  figure  12  show 
finally  that,  when  responding  at  higher  frequencies, 
the  cell  discharges  but  a  single  impulse  to  each  stimu- 
lus which  is  effective  in  contrast  to  the  repetitive  re- 
sponse to  the  first  stimulus  of  the  train.  The  repet- 
itive response  '  singles  up'  as  a  rule  when  the  fre- 
quency of  the  stimuli  increases  beyond  10  to  15  per 
sec. 

The  equilibration  type  of  response  occurs  in  about 
60  per  cent  of  the  neurons  studied  at  thalamic  and 
cortical  levels.  The  remaining  neurons  display  a 
different  sort  of  lieha\ior.  While  following  the  stimulus 


rate  up  to  values  which  differ  greatly  for  different 
units,  they  respond  to  the  presentation  of  still  faster 
trains  with  but  an  initial  response  and  are  thereafter 
silent  during  the  train,  or  discharge  randomly  at  the 
spontaneous  rate  (see  fig.  13).  It  is  not  clear  at  present 
whether  the  '  equilibration'  and  the  '  cut-off'  types  of 
response  can  be  obtained  for  the  same  unit  by  suitable 
manipulation  of  the  stimulus.  Most  of  the  units  ob- 
served which  show  the '  cut-off'  response  pattern  follow 
only  a  low  rate  of  stimulation.  On  the  other  hand,  it 
seems  to  be  true,  at  least  occasionally,  that  a  typical 
equilibration  type  of  response  becomes  a  cut-off  type 
when  the  stimulus  rate  is  made  very  high  (200  to  300 
per  sec). 

AFFERENT  INHIBITION.  It  has  been  shown  recently 
(181,  198)  that  the  afferent  volleys  evoked  by  periph- 
eral stimuli  while  excitatory  for  some  cells  of  the  sys- 
tem will  tend  to  inhibit  others  (fig.  14).  All  inhibitory 
phenomena  are  very  sensitive  to  anesthetic  agents  and 
are  probably  at  least  partially  abolished  even  under 
very  light  general  anesthesia.  Nevertheless,  the  inhibi- 
tion of  both  the  spontaneous  and  the  evoked  activity 
of  central  neurons  has  been  observed  for  a  consider- 
able number  of  cells  (198).  The  peripheral  inhibitory 
receptive  field  for  a  given  neuron  (in  the  postcentral 
cortex)  may  surround  or  lie  adjacent  to  its  excitatory 
field.  It  is  an  interesting  observation  that  a  cell 
excited,  for  example  by  movement  of  a  joint,  may  be 
inhibited  by  skin  stimulation,  although  a  purely 
excitatory  intermodality  interaction  has  not  thus  far 
been  demonstrated  for  units  driven  from  the  skin  and 
from  deep  receptors.  In  the  cortex,  pairs  of  cells  which 
are  in  one  case  excited  and  in  the  other  inhibited  from 
the  same  receptive  field  have  been  observed  at  a  single 
electrode  position.  They  must  therefore  lie  very  close 
to  one  another.  This  suggests,  of  course,  that  afferent 
inhibition  may  play  an  important  role  in  reducing 
the  discharge  zone  of  cells  activated  by  a  local  periph- 
eral stimulus.  It  need  hardly  be  added  that  restricted, 
sharply  focused  discharge  zones  may  be  instrumental 
in  recognizing  a  single  localized  peripheral  event  and 
in  more  complex  discriminations  as  well. 

suMM.\RY.  The  single  unit  studies  at  several  stations  of 
the  somatic  afferent  system  have  produced  a  consider- 
able mass  of  data  concerning  the  relation  of  a  dis- 
charge of  a  single  cell  to  the  quantitative  parameters 
of  the  peripheral  stimuli.  For  a  population  of  cells  the 
data  allow  a  reconstruction  of  the  distribution  ot 
activity  set  up  by  a  single  brief  peripheral  stimulus 
occurring  in  that  population.  However,  no  informa- 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


409 


FIG.  1 2.  Responses  of  a  single  neuron  of  ventrobasal  thalamic  nuclear  complex  of  the  cat  to  elec- 
trical stimulation  of  skin  of  the  contralateral  foreleg.  The  stimuli  were  delivered  at  diflferent  fre- 
quencies per  second  which  are  indicated  by  the  numbers  on  the  left.  Note  reduction  of  modal  \alue 
and  equilibration  of  response  with  increasing  frequencies  of  stimulation,  and  the  early  silent  period. 
Stimulus  artefacts  are  not  visible.  [From  Mountcastle,  V.  B  &  |  E.  Rose,  unpublished  observa- 
tions.] 


tion  is  yet  available  which  permits  a  complete  descrip- 
tion of  the  sequential  changes  in  neural  events  brought 
about  by  a  natural  stimulus  in  a  completelv  unanes- 
thetized  animal.  Underlying  studies  of  this  type  is  the 
assumption  that  perception  of  a  local  peripheral 
event  depends  in  the  first  instance  upon  a  local  zone 
of  cortical  activity  of  abrupt  onset,  and  that  percep- 
tion of  more  complex  forms  of  stimuli  (e.g.  two-point 
discrimination,  form  and  contoiu"  recognition,  etc.) 
may  depend  upon  the  interaction  of  many  such  zones 
of  activity.  One  of  the  problems  in  sensory  physiology 
at  the  present  time  is  to  determine  in  some  detail  the 
patterns  of  cortical  activity  evoked  by  peripheral 
stimuli  of  some  spatial  and  temporal  complexity.  It 
seems  likely  that  single  unit  studies  will  advance  the 
solution  of  this  problem. 


KINESTHESIS    OR    SENSE    OF    POSITION     .^ND 
MOVEMENTS  OF  JOINTS 

It  is  apparent  that  information  concerning  the 
orientation  of  the  bod\'  in  space  and  of  the  spatial 
relations  between  its  parts  depends  upon  afferent 
inputs  from  both  somatic  sensory  and  \estibular 
receptors  as  well  as  from  the  visual  apparatus.  The 
thesis  is  presented  here  that  the  somatic  sensory  com- 
ponent, which  we  shall  refer  to  as  kinesthesis  or  the 
sense  of  position  and  movement  of  the  joints,  depends 


FIG.  13.  Responses  of  a  single  neuron  of  the  ventrobasal 
thalamic  nuclear  complex  of  the  cat  to  electrical  stimulation  of 
the  skin  near  the  first  digit  of  the  contralateral  forepaw,  de- 
livered at  different  frequencies.  Note  reduction  of  modal  value 
as  frequency  increases  from  10  to  20  or  more  per  sec.  At 
frequency  of  40  per  sec,  or  higher,  the  neuron  responded  to  the 
first  stimulus  of  a  train  and  failed  to  respond  thereafter: 
'cut-off'  characteristic.  Small  deflections  are  stimulus  artefacts. 
[From  Mountcastle,  'V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished  observa- 
tions.] 


4IO 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


IMPULSES   PER  SECOND 

100  I- 


EXCITflTORY    STIMULUS 


^/4p-i-\^yi^ 


FIG.  14.  Interacting  effects  of  excitatory  and  inhibitory 
peripheral  stimuli  upon  the  discharge  rate  of  a  single  neuron  of 
area  i  of  the  postcentral  gyrus  of  the  monkey.  Graph  plots  the 
average  frequencies  of  discharge  of  the  neuron  in  each  succes- 
sive 400  msec,  period.  Neuron  excited  by  internal  rotation  of 
shoulder  joint,  indicated  by  upper  bar,  and  inhibited  by  pres- 
sure upon  skin  of  the  palm,  indicated  by  lower  bar;  both  contra- 
lateral. Note  that  recovery  from  inhibition  duplicates  onset  of 
excitation,  with  rapid  onset  transient  and  decline  to  a  less 
rapid  firing  level.  [From  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  T.  P.  S.  Powell, 
manuscript  in  preparation.] 


upon  the  receptor  organs  associated  with  the  joints. 
Activity  set  up  in  those  receptors  by  the  steady  posi- 
tion or  movement  of  the  joints  is  relayed  through  the 
medial  lemniscal  system,  in  a  topographical  pattern 
at  each  precortical  relay  and  in  the  somatic  sensory 
cortex  itself  which  is  mutually  interlocked  with  the 
pattern  representing  the  cutaneous  sensory  sheet. 
Since  this  is  contrary  to  the  widely  held  belief  that 
kinesthesis  depends  as  well  upon  afferent  input  from 
muscle  stretch  receptors,  the  evidence  for  it  will  be 
presented  in  some  detail. 

Mustif  Sitetiii  Receptors  and  h  uiesthests 

Evidence  accumulates  from  recent  research  that 
the  rate  of  discharge  of  the  stretch  receptors  of  muscle 
is  not  linearly  or  even  constantly  related  to  the  length 
of  the  muscle  per  se.  Since  the  classical  work  of 
Matthews  (169)  it  has  been  known  that  the  Golgi 
tendon  organs  discharge  afferent  impulses  at  a  rate 
related  to  tension.  The  tension  to  which  these  recep- 
tors are  subjected  depends  upon  the  length  of  the 
muscle,  i.e.  upon  the  joint  angle,  and  upon  the  force 
exerted  h\  the  muscle  against  its  load  h\  its  active 
contraction  which  in  turn  depends  on  the  activity  of 
the  alpha  motoneurons.  It  follows  that  the  number  of 


active  Golgi  organs  and  their  rates  of  discharge  are  not 
variables  dependent  solely  upon  the  angle  of  the  joint 
or  joints  acro.ss  which  the  muscle  works;  these  recep- 
tors cannot,  therefore,  inform  reliably  of  joint  posi- 
tion. 

The  spindle  organ  receptors  of  muscle  are  subject 
to  even  more  complex  influences.  Matthews  (169) 
had  shown  that  these  receptors  are  excited  by  stretch 
of  the  muscle  but  cease  to  discharge  as  the  muscle  is 
shortened  by  alpha  motoneuron  action.  They  may  be 
completely  silent  when  tension  at  the  tendon  is  maxi- 
mal. The  work  of  Leksell  (153)  revealed,  however, 
that  the  smaller  efferent  fibers  of  the  \entral  root, 
the  gamma  motoneurons,  produce  upon  discharge  an 
increase  in  spindle  organ  activity,  even  when  the 
muscle  shortens.  These  observations  have  been  ex- 
tended recently  (102,  129-132,  144)  and  it  is  now 
well  known  that  the  gamina  efferents  condition  affer- 
ent input  from  the  spindles  and  thus  play  an  important 
role  in  \oluntary  movement  and  reflex  regulation. 
More  recently  Granit  and  his  colleagues  (62,  103,  104) 
have  ciescriijed  the  central  nervous  control  of  the 
gamma  motoneurons  and  hence  of  spindle  organ 
discharge,  and  Eldred  &  Hagbarth  (63)  their  reflex 
regulation  bv  cutaneous  afferents.  Further  details  of 
the  function  of  the  gamma  efferent-spindle  afferent 
loop  are  presented  by  Eldred  in  Chapter  XLI  of  this 
work,  and  an  excellent  general  review  of  the  subject 
is  provided  in  the  monograph  by  Granit  (loi).  The 
important  point  in  the  present  consideration  is  that 
spindle  activitv  may  varv  from  zero  to  ma.ximum 
independenth'  of  the  length  or  tension  of  the  muscle; 
these  receptors,  like  the  Golgi  tendon  organs,  cannot 
signal  muscle  length  or  joint  angle. 

These  facts  alone  are  impressive  for  the  argument 
that  stretch  receptors  of  muscle  are  not  likely  to  in- 
form of  joint  position.  Complementary  to  them  is  the 
experimental  oiiservation  of  Lloyd  &  Mclntyre  (160) 
that  the  large  stretch  afferents  from  muscle  do  not 
project  upwards  in  the  dorsal  cokmins  but  relay  in  the 
column  of  Clarke-Stilling  into  a.scending  systems 
terminating  in  the  cerebellum.  This  observation  has 
been  confirmed  and  extended  in  an  elegant  series  of 
studies  (128,  150-152,  163)  which  showed  that  group 
I-a  afTerents  from  muscle  spindle  organs  relay  into  the 
dorsal  spinocerebellar  tract.  In  addition,  O.scarsson 
(190)  has  reported  that  group  I-b  fibers  from  tendon 
organs  project  upon  the  cells  of  origin  of  the  ventral 
spinocerebellar  tract.  Complementary  also  are  the 
negative  observations  that  direct  stretch  of  muscle 
produces    no   detectable  response  in   the  postcentral 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


homologue  of  the  cerebral  cortex  (182).  Nor  have 
single  unit  analysis  studies  revealed  any  cells  at  levels 
of  thalamus  or  cortex  which  could  he  activated  by 
stretch  of  muscle  (181;  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  T,  P.  S. 
Powell,  manuscript  in  preparation;  and  Mount- 
castle, V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished  observations). 

Data  obtained  from  experiments  in  which  the 
system  is  activated  by  electrical  stimulation  of  bared 
muscle  nerves  are  somewhat  discordant.  Mount- 
castle et  al.  (182)  reported  that  when  the  afferent 
volley  was  confined  to  group  I  afferent  fiijers  (which 
innervate  the  annulospiral  endings  and  the  Golgi 
tendon  organs),  no  responses  were  evoked  in  the  post- 
central homologue  in  anesthetized  animals.  Nor  were 
such  responses  observed  when  the  stimulus  strength 
was  increased  to  activate  group  II  afferents  (which 
innervate  the  flower  spray  stretch  receptors).  They 
did  observe,  again  in  anesthetized  animals,  that  when 
group  III  fibers  of  the  muscle  nerves  were  activated 
cortical  responses  of  long  latency  appeared.  The 
peripheral  endings  of  group  III  afferents  are  thought 
to  lie  bare  nerve  terminals  and  there  is  no  evidence 
that  they  are  sensitive  to  mechanical  changes  in  the 
muscle;  it  seems  safe  to  assume  that  these  endings  are 
of  no  significance  for  position  sense.  Perhaps  they, 
together  with  the  C-fiber  afferents,  mediate  the  sensa- 
tions of  muscle  fatigue  and  pain.  These  observations 
are  in  agreement  with  the  observation  that  direct 
stretch  of  muscle  evokes  no  detectable  response  in 
the  cerebral  cortex. 

Some  workers  (84,  86,  171)  find,  on  the  other  hand, 
that  cortical  responses  do  appear  when  the  afferent 
volley  is  thought  to  contain  the  group  II  and  possibly 
group  I  components.  Perhaps  these  discordant  results 
are  due  to  the  different  muscle  nerves  used,  for  it  is 
known  that  some  fibers  from  joint  receptors,  perios- 
teum and  deep  fascia,  may  travel  in  some  muscle 
nerves  and  are  of  group  II  size  (76,  225,  231-233). 
Whatever  the  final  answer,  it  is  clear  that  muscle 
stretch  afferents  are  unlikely  to  play  a  role  in  position 
sense  for  they  are  under  control  of  the  gamma  efferent 
loop  and  may  discharge  over  their  full  frequency 
range  at  any  muscle  length. 

Innervation  of  Joints 

The  tissues  in  and  aijout  the  joints  clearly  receive 
a  rich  innervation;  [the  older  literature  on  this  matter 
has  been  reviewed  by  Skoglund  (225)].  This  innerva- 
tion, the  receptor  organs  in  the  ligaments  and  the 
joint  capsules,  and   the  functional  properties  of  the 


receptors  have  been  intensively  studied  in  recent 
vears.  The  articular  innervation  in  a  variety  of  animals 
and  in  man  has  been  described  by  a  number  of 
workers  (16,  18,  28,  29,  75-81,  215,  216,  225,  279). 
Afferent  fibers  from  some  joints  have  been  shown  to 
travel  in  both  muscle  and  cutaneous  nerves.  The 
myelinated  fibers  vary  in  size  between  2  and  16  n 
and  according  to  Gardner  (76)  the  spectruin  displays 
definite  peaks  between  2  to  5  and  7  to  10  /i,  while 
Skoglund's  measurements  (225)  suggest  a  unimodal 
distribution  around  a  peak  at  about  3  to  6  ju.  Articular 
nerves  contain  large  numbers  of  unmyelinated  fibers, 
some  of  sympathetic  origin,  while  others  are  un- 
doubtedly afferent  dorsal  root  C  fibers.  It  is  clear 
then  that  articular  nerves  resemble  in  composition 
purely  cutaneous  ones. 

Joint  Receptors  and  Their  Discharge  Patterns 

Some  recent  histological  studies  indicate  three  types 
of  receptor  organs  in  articular  tissue  (16,  18,  28,  76). 


FIG.  15.  Graph  of  the  impulse  frequency  of  a  single  afferent 
neuron  innervating  the  capsule  of  the  knee  joint  of  the  cat. 
Graph  plots  frequency  of  impulses  against  time  as  the  joint  is 
moved  through  10  degrees  of  flexion  and  back  again,  as  in- 
dicated by  the  dashed  line.  Note  onset  transient  during  move- 
ment, adaptation  to  a  more  or  less  steady  frequency  of  discharge 
during  steady  joint  displacement,  rapid  drop  in  frequency  when 
joint  moves  away  from  excitatory  position,  postexcitatory  silent 
period,  recovery  to  resting'  frequency  of  discharge,  and  almost 
exact  repetition  of  the  pattern  of  discharge  when  the  move- 
ment is  repeated.  [From  Boyd  &  Roberts  (29).] 


412 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOC3Y 


FIG.  i6.  Lejl:  Graphs  of  the  impulse  frequency  in  a  single  aflTercnt  fiber  innervating  the  capsule  of 
the  knee  joint  of  the  cat,  showing  frequency  of  discharge  against  time  during  flexion  at  a  rate  of  lo 
degrees  per  sec.  carried  through  three  different  angles:  open  triangles,  lo  degrees;  open  circles,  12 
degrees;  closed  circles,  14  degrees.  The  upper  curves  show  the  frequencies  of  the  impulses,  the  lower 
ones  the  angular  displacements  from  a  position  of  1 32  degrees  of  extension,  where  this  receptor  did 
not  discharge.  Note  that  steady  state  frequency  is  higher  for  greater  joint  displacements.  Right: 
Similar  graphs  for  the  same  afferent  neuron  during  movements  of  the  joint  between  the  same  posi- 
tions at  four  different  rates :  closed  triangles,  35  degrees  per  sec. ;  closed  circles,  1 7  degrees  per  sec. ; 
open  triangles,  10  degrees  per  sec;  open  circles,  6  degrees  per  sec.  The  displacements  are  indicated 
by  thin  lines.  Note  that  while  onset  transients  differ,  the  steady  impulse  frequency  in  the  final  posi- 
tion is  the  same  in  each  case.  [From  Boyd  &  Roberts  (29).] 


By  far  the  most  common  are  the  '  spray-type'  endings 
which  resemble  those  described  in  the  skin  by  Ruffini. 
They  are  located  in  the  connective  tissue  capsule  of 
the  joints  but  not  in  its  synovial  lining  membrane  and 
are  supplied  by  myelinated  fibers  ranging  in  diameter 
from  7  to  10  /i  (225)-  They  are  well  fitted  by  location 
and  response  properties  to  signal  the  steady  position 
of  the  joint  and  the  direction,  rate  and  extent  of  joint 
movement  (29,  225).  They  respond  at  low  threshold 
with  a  rapid  onset  transient  as  the  joint  moves  in  a 
direction  which  causes  their  excitation  (fig.  15).  The 
rate  of  discharge  during  the  movement  is  a  function 
of  its  speed  and  extent  (fig.  16);  the  steady  state  of 
discharge  at  a  given  excitatory  displacement  is  inde- 
pendent of  the  rate  at  which  the  initial  displacement 
occurred  (fig.  i  7). 

These  .slowly  adapting  receptors  .subserve  angles  of 
aljout  15  degrees.  For  any  given  joint  different  mem- 
bers of  the  population  of  receptors  have  their  excita- 
tory angles  located   at  different  positions  along  the 


range  of  joint  movement.  Some  have  excitatory  angles 
placed  at  one  end  of  this  range,  responding  at  maximal 
rate  at  either  full  flexion,  or  full  extension  (figs.  17, 
18).  At  least  that  is  true  for  the  knee  joint  of  the  cat 
which  has  been  most  intensively  studied,  but  there  is 
no  reason  to  believe  that  qualitatively  different  condi- 
tions exist  in  other  joints  or  other  species,  including 
man,  for  the  articular  innervation  has  been  found  to 
be  remarkably  uniform  in  all  species  studied. 

A  second  slowly  adapting  receptor  resembling  in 
appearance  the  Golgi  tendon  organ  has  been  found 
associated  with  the  ligaments  of  the  joints,  and  has 
been  found  to  be  innervated  by  fibers  7  to  10  fj  in 
diameter.  This  type  is  much  less  numerous  than  the 
Ruffini  type  endings  described  above  and  possesses 
similar  discharge  properties  (16,  225).  Very  rarely, 
first  order  afferents  are  observed  which  adapt  very 
quickly  to  joint  movement  which  e.xcites  them.  Al- 
though some  disagreement  exists  (76)  they  are  thought 
to    arise    from    modified    \'ater-Pacinian    corpuscles 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS  4I3 


M 


40 


30 


20 


J L_l I I L 


-i50 


to 


30 


20 


-10 


J I I I I I I 


SO 


60 


70 


90 


90 


100 


w 


120 


130        140        ISO        160        170        180 
degrees 


FIG.  17  Lefl:  Graphs  of  the  impulse  frequency  in  single  afferent  neuron  innervating  the  capsule 
of  the  knee  joint  of  the  cat,  as  the  joint  is  moved  in  steps  through  the  'excitatory  angle'  for  the 
receptor,  in  opposite  directions.  After  each  small  step  of  the  movement  the  frequency  is  allowed 
to  reach  an  adapted  rate.  The  two  curves  are  almost  mirror  images.  [From  Skoglund  (225).] 

FIG.  18  Right:  Graphs  of  impulse  frequencies  for  eight  single  neurons  innervating  slowly  adapting  re- 
ceptors in  the  capsule  of  the  knee  joint  of  the  cat.  The  adapted  impulse  frequency  is  plotted  against 
position  of  the  joint  in  degrees.  Solid  lines  show  values  for  five  units  in  one  experiment,  dolled  lines 
show  those  for  three  units  in  another.  The  figure  is  not  fully  representative  for  the  distribution  of 
endings  which  are  successively  activated  during  full  movement  since  in  general  endings  which  cause 
maximal  adapted  discharge  rates  are  more  numerous  immediately  before  or  at  full  flexion  or  full 
extension  than  in  the  intermediate  positions  of  the  joint.  The  sensitive  ranges  (15  to  30  degrees)  are 
representative  of  the  behavior  of  most  endings.  [From  Skoglund  (aas).] 


located  in  the  pericapsular  connective  tissue  (225). 
They  are  innervated  by  the  largest  afferents  in  the 
articular  nerves. 

Central  Projection  of  Joint  Afferents 

The  evidence  that  receptors  in  and  about  joints 
do  indeed  project  into  the  lemniscal  system  was  ob- 
tained  by  gross  electrode  recording  of  the  electrical 


responses  evoked  in  the  ventrobasal  thalamic  complex 
and  the  somatic  sensory  cortex  by  mechanical  stimu- 
lation of  those  tissues  (182)  and  by  electrical  stimula- 
tion of  articular  nerves  (83,  86).  Such  experiments 
indicate  that  the  afferents  from  bones  and  joints  form 
together  with  afferents  from  cutaneous  receptors  a 
common  topographic  pattern.  Knowledge  of  this 
projection  has  been  greatly  extended  by  single  unit 
studies.  Some  single  elements  in  the  ventral  thalamic 


414 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


nucleus  and  in  the  postcentral  honiologue  are  acti- 
vated by,  and  only  by,  movement  of  the  joints  (i8i; 
and  Mountcastle,  \'.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished 
observations).  The  great  majority  of  these  neurons 
respond  not  only  to  transient  rotation  of  the  joint  to 


IMPULSES    PER    SECOND 
50 


CORTICAL    NEURON     RESPONDING  TO 
MAINTAINED    ROTATION   OF   SHOULDER  JOINT 


FIG.  ig.  Impulse  frequency  of  a  single  neuron  of  area  i  of  the 
postcentral  gyrus  of  the  macaque,  plotted  continuously  as 
average  frequency  in  each  400  msec,  period.  Neuron  is  a  deep 
joint  unit.  At  onset  of  rotation  of  the  contralateral  shoulder 
joint  there  is  a  rapid  rise  in  discharge  frequency  which  declines 
slightly  to  a  more  or  less  steady  rate  of  discharge  during 
steadily  maintained  joint  rotation  (period  of  stimulation  in- 
dicated by  black  bar).  Note  rapid  fall  of  frequency  when  joint 
is  returned  to  its  neutral  position,  and  the  postexcitatory  period 
of  low  frequency  discharge.  [From  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  T.  P.  S. 
Powell,  manuscript  in  preparation.] 


which  they  are  related  but  continue  to  discharge 
impulses  steadily  when  the  joint  is  held  within  the 
excitatory  angle,  subserved  by  the  unit  (.see  figs.  19, 
20).  They  adapt  as  a  rule  very  slowly.  The  onset 
transient  is  a  function  of  the  degree  and  rate  of  joint 
movement;  the  subsequent  steady  state  of  activity  is 
a  function  of  joint  angle  only.  In  the  cortex,  as  in  the 
periphery,  excitatory  angles  for  different  neurons 
related  to  a  given  joint  are  different,  and  some  units 
of  the  group  are  active  at  any  joint  position.  Phasic 
joint  movements  may  recruit  additional  neurons, 
increase  the  discharge  frequency  in  some  neurons 
already  active  and  decrease  it  in  still  others.  From 
this  description  a  generality  is  once  again  confirmed; 
the  discharge  patterns  of  central  neurons  of  the  lemnis- 
cal  system  arc  determined  by  those  ot  the  peripheral 
receptors  to  which  the)-  are  linked. 

It  is  an  observation  of  interest  that  pairs  of  closely 
related  cells  in  the  cerebral  cortex  may  be  reciprocally 
related  to  a  given  joint.  One  of  the  pair  is  active  and 
the  other  silent  as  the  joint  moves  in  one  direction,  and 
the  reverse  occurs  when  the  movement  alternates 
(181)  (fig.  20).  Whether  the  reciprocity  is  due  to 
some  central  reciprocal  inhibitory  exent  or  simply  to 
alternate  loading  and  unloading  of  appropriate  groups 
of  receptors  on  the  two  sides  of  the  joint  is  unknown. 
In  any  case,  it  is  likely  that  such  a  mutual  interaction 
could  serve  to  increase  the  discriminatory  capacity  in 
respect  to  the  rate  and  extent  of  joint  mo\ement. 


90  - 

o 

■z.    80 
O 
a    70 

q:    60 
uj 

'^    50 
tn 

i  30 

-    20 


10 


Reciprocolly   Responding   Cortical  Cells,  Recorded    Simultaneously 

Driven    Respectively  by  flexiono — o  ond  extension  •--• 
of  Contralateral    Elbow 


Peripherol   Receptors    Within  Elbow   Joint 


I 


\ 


FIG.  20.  Impulse  frequency  graphs  of  two  neurons  of  postcentral  homologue  of  the  cerebral  cortex 
of  the  cat.  Discharges  of  the  two  units  observed  simultaneously  at  a  single  microelcctrodc  position. 
Units  responded  reciprocally  to  alternating  flexions  and  extensions  of  the  contralateral  elbow. 
Graphs  plot  continuously  the  average  frequencies  for  each  consecutive  400  msec,  period.  Impulse 
frequency  reaches  zero  for  each  unit  when  the  joint  reaches  the  position  maximally  excitatory  for  the 
other  unit.  During  fourteenth  and  fifteenth  seconds  the  joint  was  held  in  steady  extension,  and  the 
extension  unit  fires  steadily,  while  the  flexion  unit  is  almost  completely  silent.  [From  Mountcastle 
(.81).] 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


415 


The  thesis  that  the  sense  of  position  and  movements 
of  the  joints  is  dependent  upon  joint  receptors  them- 
selves fits  well  the  clinical  observations.  However,  we 
shall  not  discuss  here  the  extensive  clinical  and  psycho- 
physical data  available  on  this  subject;  we  refer  the 
interested  reader  to  a  series  of  articles  by  Goldscheider 
collected  in  a  book  (99).  His  careful  work  and  his  clear 
recognition  of  the  joints  as  the  source  for  kinesthetic 
sensations  are  unfortunately  usually  forgotten  or  dis- 
regarded in  modern  physiological  texts  perhaps 
because  he  was  satisfied  to  treat  these  sensations  within 
the  concept  of 'muscle  sense'  (^Muskelsinn). 

Projections  of  Deij)  Receptors  Other  Than  in  Joints 

Studies  of  the  modality  properties  of  individual 
neurons  of  the  lemniscal  system  at  thalamic  and 
cortical  levels  have  shown  a  class  of  cells  whose 
peripheral  receptive  fields  lie  in  deep  fascia  (181; 
and  Mountcastle,  V.  B.  &  J.  E.  Rose,  unpublished 
observations).  These  fields  are  of  similar  shape  and 
size  as  those  for  neurons  driven  from  the  skin  (see  fig. 
7).  The  units  are  driven  by  very  light  mechanical 
stimuli  to  the  fascia,  and  the  threshold  for  activity  is 
so  low  that  even  a  very  small  displacement  of  the 
overlying  skin  may  evoke  their  discharge.  They  may 
also  be  driven  by  pressure  changes  occurring  in  the 
deep  fascial  compartments  when  the  enclosed  muscles 
contract.  It  is  reasonable  to  assume  that  this  cla.ss  of 
neurons  functions  in  the  overall  sense  of  touch-pres- 
sure, for  they  respond  to  all  but  the  very  weakest  of 
stimuli  impinging  upon  the  skin  overlying  their  own 
fascial  receptive  fields. 

Other  neurons  of  this  class  are  activated  by  direct 
pressure  upon  the  periosteum.  What  role  they  may 
play  in  kinesthesis  or  some  other  aspect  of  deep  sensi- 
bility is  unknown. 


FUNCTIONAL  ORG.^NIZ.^TION  OF  FIRST  SOMATIC 
CORTICAL    FIELD 

One  of  the  central  problems  of  neurophysiology  at 
the  present  time  is  to  understand  the  functional 
mechanisms  of  any  given  region  of  the  cerebral  cortex. 
Investigators  proceed  on  the  premise  that  if  they  can 
determine  the  patterns  of  neural  activity  entering  a 
cortical  region,  the  modifications  of  those  patterns 
occurring  across  intracortical  synaptic  relays  ('inte- 
grative action')  and  the  spatial  and  temporal  patterns 
of  output  from  the  region,  they  will  then  be  able  to 
reconstruct  with  some  insight  the  way  in  which  the 
particular  cortical  region  operates.   In  the  past  few 


years  much  effort  has  been  expended  to  study  the 
response  properties  of  single  cells  in  the  first  somatic 
field,  the  way  these  cells  are  activated  from  the  thala- 
mus, the  relations  of  the  unitary  discharges  to  the 
evoked  slow  cortical  wave  on  the  surface  of  the  cortex 
and  in  its  depths  and  the  relation  of  single  cell  dis- 
charges to  the  cortical  EEG  (13,  14,  157-159),  matters 
recently  reviewed  by  Albe-Fe.ssard  (7).  Recently 
Mountcastle  (181)  has  suggested  on  the  basis  of  his 
studies  that  a  vertical  group  of  cells  extending  across 
all  the  cellular  layers  acts,  as  it  were,  as  a  functional 
cortical  unit.  Three  observations  are  the  reasons  for 
this  suggestion,  a)  The  neurons  of  such  a  vertical 
group  are  all  related  to  the  same,  or  nearly  the  same, 
peripheral  receptive  field.  This  observation  establishes 
also  that  the  topographical  pattern  present  on  the 
surface  of  the  cortex  extends  throughout  its  depth. 
6)  The  neurons  of  such  a  vertical  group  belong  as  a 
rule  to  the  same  modality  group,  i.e.  they  are  acti- 
vated by  the  same  type  of  peripheral  stimulus.  This 
implies  that  a  small  group  of  thalamocortical  fibers 
entering  the  cortex  is  activated  by  a  single  mode  of 
peripheral  stimulation  and  in  turn  activates  a  narrow 
vertical  column  of  cortical  cells.  0  All  cells  of  such  a 
vertical  column  disrharge  on  the  average  at  more  or 
less  the  same  latency  to  a  brief  peripheral  stimulus. 
The  discharges  are  thus  grouped  within  the  time  limits 
of  a  few  milliseconds  into  an  initial  firing  pattern.  This 
observation  is  based,  however,  only  upon  the  first 
response  of  cortical  cells,  the  latency  of  which  is 
known  to  be  sensitive  to  various  parameters  of  the 
peripheral  stimulus. 

The  possibility  that  a  vertical  column  of  cells  tends 
to  behave  as  a  functional  unit  appears  acceptable 
anatomically  both  from  the  cytoarchitectural  point  of 
view  and  as  regards  the  connections  of  such  a  vertical 
column  as  seen  in  the  Golgi  material  (162).  The  sub- 
pial  dicing  experiments  of  several  cortical  fields  (226, 
227)  could  also  be  interpreted  to  imply  that  a  complex 
cortical  activity  is  still  possible  as  long  as  the  cortical 
organization  in  depth  is  preserved. 

It  is,  of  course,  not  implied  by  these  observations 
that  the  cortex  is  organized  into  sets  of  isolated, 
vertically  oriented  tissue  prisms.  It  appears,  however, 
that  at  least  for  the  incoming  activity  a  columnar 
vertical  organization  is  of  special  significance. 


SPINOTHALAMIC    SYSTEM 


In  comparison  to  our  knowledge  of  the  lemniscal 
system,  that  pertaining  to  the  tactile  activity  of  the 


4i6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


spinothalamic  system  is  quite  inadequate  since  many 
basic  questions  concerning  it  are  still  not  solved.  As 
we  define  the  system  it  consists  of  the  spinothalamic 
tract  arising  in  the  posterior  horns  and  of  the  bulbo- 
thalamic  tract  originating  in  the  spinal  nucleus  of  the 
fifth  nerve. ^  The  system  is  known  to  transmit  impulses 
provoked  by  painful  and  thermal  stimuli  but  there  is 
adequate  evidence  as  well  that  some  tactile  impulses 
are  also  relayed  through  it. 

Location  of  Tactile  Fibers  m  Sjntuithalamic  System 

It  is  customary  to  distinguish  within  the  spinotha- 
lamic system  of  the  spinal  cord  (though  not  in  the 
bulbothalamic  tract)  a  ventral  and  a  lateral  spino- 
thalamic pathw-ay.  The  first  is  assumed  to  conduct 
tactile  impulses,  the  second  is  known  to  be  important 
for  arousal  of  painful  and  thermal  .sensations.  The 
ventral  spinothalamic  tract  is  usually  believed  to  lie 
in  the  medial  aspect  of  the  anterolateral  column. 
Clinical  experience  in  man  indicates  that  some  fibers 
in  this  column  must  be  concerned  with  touch  since 
tactile  anesthesia  results  only  if  in  addition  to  a  de- 
struction of  a  posterior  column  on  one  side  a  contra- 
lateral injury  is  present  .somewhere  in  the  region  of  the 
anterolateral  column.  On  the  other  hand,  only 
partial  impairment  in  tactile  sensation  occurs  when 
either  of  these  columns  is  .selectively  injured.  In  fact, 
it  has  been  frequently  believed  in  the  past  that  a 
destruction  of  the  anterolateral  column  alone  does 
not  lead  to  any  deficits  in  tactile  sensations.  Foerster 
&  Gagel  (71),  Foerster  (70)  and  KroU  (141),  however, 
using  finer  testing  techniques  were  able  to  determine 
some    such    deficits   after   anterolateral    cordotomies. 

-  Similar  to  the  uncertainty  which  prevails  in  respect  to  the 
secondary  trigeminal  pathways  arising  in  the  main  sensory 
nucleus  of  the  fifth  nerve  (sec  p.  396),  a  considerable  confusion 
and  controversy  exists  in  regard  to  the  central  course  of  the 
bulbothalamic  tract.  Wallenberg  (259)  who  described  this 
pathway  maintained  that  it  ascends  in  the  dorsolateral  portion 
of  the  reticular  substance  of  the  brain  stem  tegmentum.  He 
believed  that  it  terminates  in  the  region  of  the  centrum  medi- 
anum  and  in  the  arcuate  component  of  the  ventrobasal  com- 
plex. Biirgi's  observations  (36)  imply  that  some  of  these  fibers 
may  end  also  in  the  n.  lateralis  posterior.  Although  Wallen- 
berg's findings  as  to  the  course  of  this  tract  in  the  brain  stem 
have  been  repeatedly  confirmed  (93,  241),  many  observers 
conclude  that  the  bulbottialamic  tract  crosses  to  join  the  medial 
lemniscus  and  separates  from  it  again  at  the  level  of  the  mid- 
brain to  join  the  spinothalamic  tract.  We  believe  that  Wallen- 
berg's original  description  is  likely  to  be  correct  and  that  con- 
trary results  are  probably  due  to  lesions  involving  the  posterior 
column  nuclei.  For  a  contrary  view  and  the  review  of  the  litera- 
ture on  this  subject  see  Biirgi  (36). 


The  deficits  are  generally  described  as  an  increase  in 
threshold  for  tactile  stimuli  and  a  decrease  in  the 
number  of 'sensory  spots'  without  any  readily  detect- 
able iinpairment  in  the  capacity  to  localize  the 
stimuli  or  to  discriminate  between  them.  These  ob- 
servations have  been  confirined  at  all  levels  of  the 
spinothalamic  system  by  a  number  of  subsequent 
workers  who  were  interested  in  this  problem  (58, 
109-111,  256,  270,  274).  A  striking  aspect  of  tactile 
impairment  is  that  tickle  sensations  disappear  with 
some  lesions  of  the  anterolateral  column  and  that 
with  bilateral  lesions  severe  disturbances  of  sensations 
in  the  sexual  sphere  are  present. 

Even  though  it  is  established  that  the  spinothalamic 
system  must  relay  some  tactile  impulses,  any  exact 
definition  of  the  fibers  concerned  and  therefore  the 
very  existence  of  a  separate  ventral  spinothalamic 
tract  as  a  tactile  component  of  the  spinothalamic 
system  seems  to  be  based  mainly  on  suppositions. 
Foerster  &  Gagel  (71)  snd  Foerster  (70)  concluded 
that  fibers  concerned  with  temperature  lie  dorsally 
to  the  fibers  concerned  with  pain  in  the  lateral  spino- 
thalamic tract  and  they  assigned  on  a  hypothetical 
ijasis  the  anterior  column  to  touch  and  pressure. 
Walker  (255)  modified  this  scheme  and  believed  the 
fibers  concerned  with  touch  to  lie  in  the  most  medial 
aspects  of  the  anterolateral  column  and,  although  he 
emphasized  the  apparent  overlap,  he  retained  the 
basic  sequence  of  separate  fiber  systems  for  tempera- 
ture, pain  and  touch.  Many  recent  observers  stress 
the  apparent  or  real  overlap  of  fibers  concerned  with 
pain  and  temperature  [for  a  review  of  the  literature 
see  White  &  Sweet  (273)],  but  they  are  usually  non- 
committal on  the  problem  of  touch.  Apparently  this 
is  so  because  touch  deficits  resulting  from  anterior 
cordotomy,  or  tractotomies  performed  at  the  level  of 
the  medulla,  pons  or  midbrain  are  of  little  or  no  clini- 
cal discomfort  to  the  patients,  because  they  are  diffi- 
cult to  detect  and  evaluate  without  special  tests,  and 
because  many  observers  were  primarily  interested  in 
the  problem  of  pain.  In  consequence,  despite  the  very 
large  number  of  operati\e  procedures  performed  in 
man  on  the  spinothalamic  system  there  is  still  no  con- 
clusive evidence  as  to  whether  touch  deficits,  such  as 
they  are,  result  from  injury  of  a  separate  sector  of  the 
spinothalamic  tract  or  whether  fibers  concerned  with 
touch  are  modality  specific  but  are  intermingled  with 
other  fibers  of  the  system.  Finally,  it  is  possible  that  no 
modality  specific  tactile  fibers  exist  within  the  system. 
The  fact  that  after  anterior  cordotomies  tickle  sensa- 
tions in  the  analgesic  areas  have  been  reported  as 
alwavs  lost  (70,  71),  as  almost  always  preserved  (134) 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


4>7 


or  only  sometimes  lost  (273)  could  perhaps  be  inter- 
preted to  mean  that  at  least  some  degree  of  separation 
exists  between  the  fibers  concerned  with  touch  and 
those  relevant  for  pain.  This  could  be  expected  if  the 
different  observers  differed  in  their  routine  sectioning 
of  the  anterolateral  column  in  regard  to  the  extent 
and  the  depth  of  the  cut.  However,  more  exact  work 
is  needed  before  one  can  conclude  that  the  classical 
notions  in  respect  to  touch  hold  true  for  the  spino- 
thalamic system. 

Origin  of  Spinothalamic  System 

The  knowledge  that  large  cutaneous  fibers  are 
activated  by  mechanoreceptors  and  that  painful  and 
thermal  stimuli  usually  activate  small  fibers  was  ob- 
viously largely  responsible  for  the  deductions  regard- 
the  origin  of  the  ventral  spinothalamic  tract.  This 
tract  is  often  conceived  as  originating  solely  from  those 
posterior  horn  cells  which  themselves  are  assumed  to 
be  activated  by  the  collaterals  of  the  large  myelinated 
fibers  of  the  medial  division  of  the  posterior  root.  The 
lateral  spinothalamic  tract,  on  the  other  hand,  is 
pictured  as  originating  from  cells  which  are  dis- 
charged by  the  small  fibers  of  the  lateral  division  of 
this  root.  The  evidence  at  hand  is  clearly  discordant 
with  such  concepts  since  it  is  certain  that  tactile  stim- 
uli can  activate  A  fibers  of  different  sizes  and  since  it 
is  probable  that  even  some  C  fibers  can  be  so  activated 
as  well  (see  p.  394).  It  follows,  therefore,  that  no  cell 
in  the  posterior  horn  which  emits  an  axon  into  the 
spinothalamic  tract  can  be  excluded  at  present  as 
potentially  responsive  to  tactile  stimuli.  The  evidence 
secured  with  the  retrograde  degeneration  method  after 
cutting  the  anterolateral  column  (71,  146,  147,  178) 
implies  that  only  the  large  apical,  pericornual  and 
basal  cells  of  the  posterior  horn  give  rise  to  the  spino- 
thalamic tract.  The  findings  of  Kuru  (147)  suggest 
that  the  large  cells  just  below  the  substantia  gelatinosa 
give  rise  to  the  ventral  spinothalamic  tract  and  hence 
to  the  tactile  component  of  the  spinothalamic  system 
if,  indeed,  it  is  true  that  the  fibers  carrying  touch  are 
running  in  a  separate  sector  and  if  this  sector  lies 
ventromedially  to  the  other  fibers  of  the  system. 
Curiously  enough,  no  retrograde  degenerations 
(after  cutting  of  the  anterolateral  column)  were  ob- 
served in  the  suljstantia  gelatinosa  cells,  which  is  one 
of  the  reasons  for  suggesting  that  these  cells  may 
actually  represent  a  system  intercalated  lietween  the 
axons  of  the  posterior  roots  and  the  cells  of  origin  of 


the  spinothalamic  system  (194).  However,  more 
evidence  is  needed  to  support  this  concept  convinc- 
ingly. 

Termination  of  Spinothalamic  System 

Ever  since  the  description  of  Edinger  (61)  there  has 
been  general  agreement  (44,  47,  53,  82,  96,  100,  180, 
204,  263)  that  the  spinothalamic  tract  is  easily 
demonstrable  in  man  and  other  primates.  However, 
despite  early  descriptions  of  both  the  spinothalamic 
and  the  bulbothalamic  components  of  this  system  in 
the  rabbit  (139,  259,  260)  douljts  have  Ijeen  frequently 
expressed  as  to  whether  the  system  actually  reaches 
the  thalamus  in  forms  other  than  primates.  The  system 
is  known  to  be  composed  mainly  of  small  fibers  (1 14) 
and  it  seems  right  to  assume  that  only  a  fraction  of 
them  is  actuall)'  traceable  with  the  Marchi  technique 
usually  employed.  It  would  appear  that  the  poor 
m\elination  of  the  fibers  accounts  reasonably  for  the 
failure  of  some  workers  to  trace  the  system  to  the 
thalamus  although  recently  a  claim  has  been  made  on 
the  basis  of  studies  employing  silver  technique  (173) 
that,  in  comparison  with  the  primates,  actually 
fewer  fibers  of  the  system  reach  the  thalamus  in  sub- 
primate  forms.  In  any  case  it  seems  clear  that  the 
spinothalamic  system  reaches  the  thalamus  in  all 
mammals  studied  and  specifically  so  in  the  cat  (95, 
I  78)  which  is  used  so  frequently  in  modern  research. 
Although  some  fibers  to  the  centrum  medianum,  to 
the  parafascicular  nucleus  and  to  the  intralaminar 
nuclei  are  often  described,  most  workers  are  agreed 
that  the  system  ends  mainly  in  the  \entrobasal  com- 
plex of  the  thalamus.  The  spinal  component  is  stated 
to  end  in  the  external  element;  the  Ijulbothalamic 
component,  in  the  arcuate  element  of  this  complex. 
As  far  as  the  tactile  system  is  concerned,  the  classical 
concept  assumes  that  the  lemniscal  and  the  spino- 
thalamic systems  converge  upon  the  ventrobasal 
complex  and  that  from  here  on  corticopetal  pathways 
are  common  to  both. 

It  is  possible  that  this  concept  may  need  a  revision 
since  it  seems  appropriate  to  suggest  that  besides  the 
ventrobasal  complex  a  region  intercalated  between 
this  complex  and  the  medial  geniculate  i:)ody  may  be 
a  major  terminal  station  for  some  spinothalamic 
fibers.  The  reasons  for  this  suggestion  are  as  follows. 

Many  ol:)servers  in  the  past  have  been  greatly  dis- 
turbed by  the  scarcity  of  detectable  terminations  of 
the  spinothalamic  fibers  in  the  ventrol)asal  complex. 
It  is  clear  from  descriptions  by  most  of  the  writers 


4i8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


cited  that  the  spinothalamic  tract  can  usually  be  fol- 
lowed with  fair  ease  up  to  the  region  immediately 
medial  to  the  medial  geniculate  body.  It  is  the  area 
between  this  region  and  the  ventrobasal  complex 
itself  in  which  so  many  Marchi  granules  disappear 
with  the  result  that  the  number  of  terminations  in  the 
ventrobasal  complex  is  often  only  scanty.  The  point 
to  be  made  is  that  this  area,  which  is  morphologically 
a  part  of  the  posterior  nuclear  thalamic  group  (209), 
is  the  critical  region  under  consideration. 

In  the  cat;  this  region  will  remain  essentially  pre- 
served after  an  extensive  ablation  of  the  suprasylvian 
and  lateral  gyri  and  of  the  entire  auditory  region.  It 
will  degenerate  completely  if  in  addition  to  this  mas- 
sive remoxal  the  second  somatic  aiVa  is  ablated  as 
well.  Nevertheless,  the  removal  of  the  second  somatic 
area  alone  will  not  cause  any  marked  changes  (209). 
It  appears  then  that  the  axons  of  this  thalamic  region 
entertain  connections  (probably  of  collateral  nature) 
with  the  second  somatic  area.  This  conclusion  is 
harmonious  with  the  findings  of  Knighton  (138)  who, 
attempting  to  determine  the  thalamic  relay  nucleus 
for  the  second  somatic  area,  found  that  stimulation 
of  the  posterior  segment  of  what  he  beliexed  to  be  the 
n.  ventralis  posteromedialis  activates  the  second 
somatic  field.  From  his  drawings  one  can  be  fairly 
confident  that  the  actual  locus  of  Knighton's  critical 
area  was  that  segment  of  the  posterior  nuclear  group 
which  is  intercalated  between  the  ventrobasal  com- 
plex and  the  medial  geniculate.  A  similar  interpreta- 
tion applies,  in  our  opinion,  to  the  findings  of  Strat- 
ford (235)  who  studied  the  corticothalamic  projections 
of  the  second  somatic  area  by  means  of  the  strychnine 
technique. 

The  evidence  which  suggests  that  the  second  so- 
matic area  may  be  activated  by  a  thalamic  grouping 
other  than  the  classical  tactile  thalamic  region  agrees 
also  with  the  findings  of  Woolsey  &  Wang  (278)  and 
VVoolsey  (personal  communication)  who  determined 
that  after  ablation  of  the  first  somatic  area  in  an  acute 
or  chronic  e.xperiment  the  responses  in  the  second 
somatic  area  are  not  detectably  affected. 

It  is  tempting  to  assume  that  the  second  somatic 
area  is  acti\ated  solely  by  the  spinothalamic  system, 
an  assumption  which,  if  true,  could  shed  new  light  on 
the  function  of  this  cortical  region.  This  assumption, 
however,  implies  that  a  destruction  of  both  antero- 
lateral columns  should  eliminate  potentials  evoked  by 
tactile  stimuli  in  the  second  somatic  cortex.  While  the 
evidence  in  this  respect  is  scanty,  the  findings  at  hand 
imply  that  this  is  not  the  ca.se. 


Topical  Orgariiz'iti'in  (if  Spinothalamic  System 

We  have  already  indicated  that  the  a\ailable  evi- 
dence is  inconclusive  for  deciding  whether  tactile 
impulses  are  relayed  within  the  spinothalamic  system 
in  a  separate  spectrum  of  fibers  or  whether  they  are 
transmitted  partly  or  wholly  by  neurons  which  are 
also  utilized  by  discharges  provoked  by  painful  or 
thermal  stimuli.  The  existence  of  a  specific  tactile 
pathway  is  inferred  primarily  from  the  observations 
that  thermal  and  painful  .sensations  may  be  affected 
differentially  by  lesions  of  the  spinal  cord  [for  review 
of  literature  see  White  &  Sweet  (273)].  The  observa- 
tions that  such  dissociations  are  not  easily  produced 
even  with  shallow  incisions  into  the  spinothalamic 
tract  and  that  they  are  altogether  rare  or  nonexistent 
with  extensive  anterolateral  cordotomies  do  not 
militate,  we  believe,  against  the  concept  of  separate 
pain  and  temperature  pathways  as  is  occasionally 
argued. 

Regardless  of  how  the  spinothalamic  system  may  be 
organized  in  respect  to  different  modalities  of  sensa- 
tions, the  evidence  is  conclusive  that  it  is  topically 
organized  in  respect  to  the  body  surface.  Thus,  the 
dermatomes  are  described  as  projecting  in  an  orderly 
fashion  upon  the  cells  of  substantia  gelatinosa  (237). 
It  has  been  deduced  early  (196)  and  since  amply  con- 
firmed by  virtually  all  who  perform  anterolateral 
cordotomies  that  fibers  concerned  with  the  caudal 
portions  of  the  body  lie  laterally  to  those  related  to 
more  oral  skin  areas  at  any  level  of  the  spinal  cord. 
The  same  basic  sequence  prevails  in  the  medulla  (57, 
218,  219,  271),  apparently  in  the  pons,  in  the  mid- 
brain (257)  and  in  respect  to  the  terminations  in  the 
thalamus  (44,  47,  263)  although  the  details  may  vary 
somewhat  at  different  levels. 

Likewise,  a  topical  organization  of  the  trigeminal 
fibers  is  well  established.  The  anatomical  evidence 
indicates  that  the  fibers  of  the  mandibular,  the  maxil- 
lary and  the  ophthalmic  divisions  of  the  fifth  nerve 
are  arranged  in  a  dorsoventral  sequence  in  the  spinal 
tract  [for  reviews  of  the  literature  .see  Astrom  (19)  and 
Torvik  (23B)],  and  this  sequence  has  been  confirmed 
by  electrophvsiological  studies  as  well  (172).  In  man, 
the  topical  organization  of  this  tract  was  inferred  on 
the  basis  of  clinical  observations  (234)  and  these 
deductions  were  proved  substantially  correct  when 
pain-relieving  operations  were  introduced.  Although 
the  opinions  are  not  unanimous  (64,  109,  iii,  115, 
133,  188,  202,  224),  it  is  probable  that  in  man,  as  in 
others  mammals,  there  is  also  a  topical  organization 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


4'9 


of  three  trigeminal  divisions  in  an  orocaudal  sequence. 
The  fibers  of  the  mandit)ular  division  do  not  reach 
as  far  caudally  as  do  some  fibers  of  the  maxillary 
division  nor  these  as  far  as  do  some  fibers  of  the 
ophthalmic  division.  The  clinical  experience  of  some 
observers  (32,  273)  led  them  to  belie\e  that  the  spinal 
tract  of  the  fifth  nerve  must  be  joined  by  some  fibers 
relevant  for  pain  and  temperature  sensations  from  the 
vagal,  glossopharyngeal  and  intermedius  nerves. 
While  this  inference  needs  further  confirmation,  it 
seems  likely  to  be  correct.  The  evidence  in  respect  to 
the  ijulbothalamic  tract  is  limited.  Some  data,  how- 
ever, clearly  suggest  that  a  topical  organization  of  its 
fibers  exists  here  as  elsewhere  in  the  spinothalamic 
system  (55). 

A  statement  that  the  spinothalamic  system  is 
topically  organized,  but  that  nevertheless  considerable 
intermingling  of  fibers  related  to  different  segments  of 
the  skin  takes  place,  seems  a  fair  reflection  of  the 
opinions  of  the  majority  of  neurosurgeons  (272).  This 
conclusion  is  based  primarily  on  the  common  experi- 
ence that  shallow  cuts  into  the  spinothalamic  tract 
tend  to  produce  only  transient  analgesias  of  higher 
segmental  levels  of  the  body  and  that  in  order  to 
secure  lasting  and  complete  effects  a  section  as  deep  as 
practicable  is  usually  necessary.  If  the  fibers  concerned 
with  pain  for  all  parts  of  the  body  were  indeed  known 
to  lie  quite  superficially,  such  observations  would 
constitute  a  proof  for  the  existence  of  extensive  over- 
lap. In  fact,  however,  the  topographical  position  of 
such  fibers  is  quite  obscure.  If  they  should  lie  deep, 
and  if  the  transient  symptomatology  with  shallow- 
cuts  results  basically  from  contusion  or  compression 
and  not  transection  of  the  relevant  fibers,  the  actual 
o\erlap  could  be  quite  small.  The  important  point  is 
that  the  data  in  respect  to  the  spinal  trigeminal  tract 
suggest  that  its  topical  organization  is  quite  precise. 
If  the  clinical  evidence  regarding  extensive  overlap 
is  not  considered  binding  there  is  hardly  any  reason 
to  suppose  that  the  entire  spinothalamic  system  is 
necessarily  less  precisely  organized  than  is  the  system 
of  the  medial  lemniscus. 

Ipsilateral  Pathways  of  Spinothalamic  System 

For  many  decades  it  has  been  a  belief,  and  this  view 
is  still  held  frequently,  that  all  the  fibers  of  the  spino- 
thalamic tract  originating  in  the  cells  of  the  posterior 
horn  cross  to  the  opposite  side  and  ascend  within  the 
anterolateral  column.  With  the  introduction  of  cordot- 
omy operations  the  view  was  advanced  that  in  addi- 
tion to  a  contralateral  tract  an  ipsilateral  component 


of  the  spinothalamic  system  exists.  Foerster  &  Gagel 
(71)  and  Foerster  (70)  advocated  this  view  and  gave 
several  arguments  for  their  belief  The  most  cogent 
were  two  observations:  first,  retrograde  changes  in 
some  cells  of  the  posterior  horn  occur  on  the  side  of 
the  cord  lesion  (in  addition  to  widespread  changes  on 
the  contralateral  .side);  and  second,  .some  deficits  of 
tactile  (and  pain)  sensations  are  demonstrable  on  the 
side  of  the  operation. 

Anatomically  there  are  .some  reasons  to  believe  that 
an  uncrossed  spinothalamic  tract  exists,  for  some 
cells  of  the  posterior  horn  emitting  a.xons  into  the 
anterolateral  column  of  the  same  side  have  iieen 
described  (27,  156,  201)  and  the  occurrence  of  ipsi- 
lateral retrograde  changes  after  appropriate  cord 
lesions  has  been  confirmed  (147,  178).  The  observa- 
tion that  electrical  stimulation  of  anterolateral  column 
may  evoke  pain  on  the  same  side  (236)  and  the  rare 
occurrence  of  pain  which  is  relieved  by  an  ipsilateral 
cordotomy  militate  for  the  existence  of  the  ipsilateral 
tract.  Since  the  electrophysiological  evidence  in  the 
cat  and  monkey  is  concordant  with  the  findings  in 
man  it  can  be  inferred  that  an  ipsilateral  as  well  as  a 
contralateral  spinothalamic  tract  presumably  exists 
in  all  mammals. 


SOME  FURTHER  OBSERV.JiTIONS  ON  SOMATIC 
SENSORY  SYSTEM 

Relaying  of  Somatic  Afferent  Impulses 

If  the  reasonable  assumption  is  made  that  it  is  via 
the  main  sensory  afferent  pathways  that  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  sensory  nerve  evokes  early  responses 
in  the  first  and  second  somatic  cortical  fields  it  appears 
a  simple  matter  to  determine  the  location  of  the 
relevant  paths  in  the  cord  or  brain  stem  by  appro- 
priately placed  lesions  in  these  structures.  However 
simple  this  method  may  be  in  principle  it  has  proved 
itself  quite  difficult  in  practice  for  it  yielded  occasion- 
ally contradictory  results  in  the  hands  of  difTerenl 
workers  and  often  inconsistent  results  in  the  hands  of 
the  same  observers. 

Thus,  Bohm  (25)  and  Bohm  &  Petersen  (26) 
found  in  their  experiments  that  selective  sectioning 
of  the  posterior  columns  abolished  responses  in  the 
somatic  areas  I  and  II  (SI  and  SII).  Bohm  concluded 
rather  boldly  that  the  discharges  in  the  anterolateral 
column  are  not  relayed  to  the  cortex.  Gardner  and 
his  colleagues  (83-86),  on  the  other  hand,  who  de- 
voted much  work  to  this  problem,  arrived  at  conclu- 


420 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


sions  much  more  in  accord  with  other  data  ahhough 
some  of  their  findings  are  puzzhng.  First  of  all,  they 
established  that  the  potentials  evoked  in  the  contra- 
lateral SI  and  SII  (contralateral  in  respect  to  the 
stimulated  nerve)  can  be  relayed  both  through  the 
ipsilateral  dorsal  column  and  through  the  contra- 
lateral anterolateral  one.  This  is,  of  course,  expected 
from  the  classical  studies.  However,  such  potentials 
seem  to  be  relayed  as  well  through  the  anterolateral 
column  on  the  side  of  the  stimulated  nerve.  While 
electrophysiological  evidence  concerning  the  existence 
of  an  ipsilateral  pathway  is,  as  already  mentioned, 
quite  in  harmony  w'ith  other  data,  the  finding  that 
the  discharges  in  this  tract  can  relay  also  to  the  contra- 
lateral cortex  is  anatomically  not  at  all  self-evident. 
Thus,  despite  some  ofjservations  to  the  contrary  it 
must  be  assumed  that  destruction  of  an  antero- 
lateral column  causes  only  ipsilateral  terminal  degen- 
erations in  the  thalamus.  Likewise  it  is  doubtful 
despite  repeated  affirmative  reports  that  spinotha- 
lamic fibers  actually  cross  partly  within  the  posterior 
commissure.  The  latter  doubt  is  reinforced  by  findings 
of  Gardner  &  Morin  (85)  which  imply  that  all  cross- 
ings of  sensory  paths  must  take  place  below  the  mid- 
brain. It  appears  then  that,  if  the  contralateral  cortex 
is  indeed  activated  via  the  anterolateral  column  of 
the  same  side  as  the  stimulated  nerve,  the  route  by 
which  this  takes  place  is  yet  to  be  determined. 

In  preparations  in  which  the  anterolateral  column 
of  the  same  side  was  the  only  column  available  for 
conduction  the  contralateral  cortical  potentials  were 
sometimes  not  evokable,  and  if  evoked  sometimes 
displayed  longer  latencies  than  in  the  intact  animals 
(83).  This  suggests  perhaps  that  other  than  the 
classical  pathways  might  have  been  involved  in  the 
transmission  of  the  discharges. 

There  is  only  fragmentary  information  available  in 
regard  to  the  ipsilateral  responses  in  SII.  It  might  be 
reasonable  to  expect  that  such  responses  are  con- 
ducted through  the  ipsilateral  spinothalamic  path- 
way. However,  out  of  three  animals  (83)  in  which  this 
path  was  presumably  cut  the  ipsilateral  cortical 
responses  disappeared  in  only  one.  Clearly,  more  data 
are  needed  before  any  conclusions  can  be  drawn. 

The  question  of  the  existence  of  an  ipsilateral  path- 
way from  the  nuclei  of  the  posterior  column  to  the 
ventrobasal  complex  aroused  considerable  interest. 
Several  workers  (26,  51,  116)  using  sectioning  tech- 
niques concluded  that  such  a  pathway  exists.  How- 
ever, in  our  opinion  both  the  anatomical  and  the 
functional  evidence  (see  p.  400)  implies  rather  defi- 
nitelv  that  such  conclusions  must  have  been  in  error. 


The  problem  of  whether  discharges  evoked  by 
stimulation  of  the  afferent  nerve  may  ascend  within 
the  spinal  cord  by  other  than  the  dorsal  column  or 
spinothalamic  pathways  has  been  considered  re- 
peatedly (41,  91,  176)  and  some  evidence  has  been 
adduced  that  this  may  be  the  case.  Morin  (176)  pro- 
posed that  one  such  pathway  runs  in  the  dorsal  part 
of  the  lateral  column  and  that  it  synapses  in  the  nu- 
cleus cervicalis  lateralis.  A  crossing  to  the  other  side 
is  believed  to  take  place  at  the  upper  cervical  levels. 
N.  cervicalis  lateralis,  however,  has  been  asserted 
(205,  206)  to  relay  exclusively  to  the  cerebellum,  a 
finding    strongly    contested    by   Morin    &    Catalano 

(177)- 

In  summary  it  appears  that  observations  of  evoked 
potentials  after  sectioning  various  fiber  tracts  have 
yielded  thus  far  only  limited  data.  The  important 
finding  is  that  impulses  from  both  sides  of  the  body 
are  conducted  in  each  anterolateral  column.  Some 
equivocal  or  contradictory  results  may  be  due  in  part 
to  the  difficulties  in  distinguishing  in  acute  experi- 
ments the  shock  due  to  the  acute  lesion  itself  from  the 
results  of  destruction  of  the  relevant  fibers.  It  is  also 
possible  that  a  massive  electrical  stimulation  of  the 
nerve  might  contribute  to  some  confusing  findings. 
Whatever  the  reasons  may  be  for  the  difficulties 
encountered,  much  more  systematic  work  is  needed 
with  this  technique. 

Centrijugal  Pathiva\s  Impinging  Upon  Sensory 
Somatic  Synaptic  Regions 

It  is  implicit  in  many  present  day  concepts  concern- 
ing the  organization  of  the  central  nervous  system  that 
synaptic  regions  situated  orally  in  a  pohsynaptic 
chain  of  an  afferent  system  are  capable  of  modulating 
the  \ery  inflow  which  arouses  their  activity.  However 
well  founded  such  ideas  may  be,  no  rigorous  proof  has 
thus  far  been  offered  for  the  existence  of  such  circuits 
in  the  somatic  sensory  system,  although  recently 
suggestive  evidence  to  this  effect  has  been  ad\anced. 
Thus,  Brodal  et  al.  (34)  and  Walberg  (248)  have 
described  direct  Ijilateral  corticofugal  connections  to 
the  sensory  trigeminal  elements  and  to  the  gracile  and 
cuneate  nuclei,  arising  not  only  from  the  sensorimotor 
area  but  from  all  major  cortical  regions  as  well.  The 
former  connections  would  clearly  represent  a  '  feed- 
back' system.  It  is  certainly  unexpected  that  virtually 
all  cortical  regions  should  affect  the  essential  compo- 
nents of  the  projectional  tactile  system  in  a  basically 
identical  fashion.  The  lack  of  any  somatotopical 
organization  of  the  projection  arising  in  the  somatic 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


421 


fields  is  puzzling  since  all  synaptic  regions  of  the 
lemniscal  system  including  the  postcentral  cortex  are 
in  fact  organized  topically  quite  precisely. 

Electrophysiological  evidence  indicates  that  repeti- 
tive electrical  stimulation  of  the  sensoriinotor  cortex 
may  depress  the  postsynaptic  response  which  is 
evoked  in  the  following  regions:  a)  in  the  trigeminal 
nucleus  by  stimulation  of  the  infraorbital  nerves  (i  25); 
b')  in  the  posterior  column  nuclei  by  stimulation  of  the 
posterior  columns  (217);  and  (r)  in  the  anterolateral 
column  by  stimulation  of  the  contralateral  dorsal 
roots  (112).  A  destruction  of  the  midbrain  reticular 
formation  (presumably  the  destruction  of  the  mid- 
brain tegmentum),  on  the  other  hand,  was  observed 
to  enhance  the  postsynaptic  response  in  the  trigeminal 
nucleus  when  the  infraorbital  nerve  was  stimulated 
(126).  In  harmony  with  the  latter  observation  King 
et  al.  (137)  observed  recentK'  that  responses  recorded 
in  the  internal  capsule  to  stimulation  of  the  peripheral 
nerve  displayed  a  reduced  amplitude  but  also  a  de- 
crease of  the  latent  period  when  an  EEG  arousal  was 
induced  by  repetitive  electrical  stimulation  of  the 
sciatic  nerve,  of  the  midbrain  reticular  formation  or 
of  the  centrum  medianum  and  n.  centralis  lateralis. 

How  to  interpret  these  findings  may  be  left  an  open 
question  since,  except  for  the  work  of  King  el  al.  (137), 
no  quantitative  data  have  been  thus  far  offered  to 
substantiate  an  effect  which  manifests  itself  bv  influ- 
encing the  test  response  only  quantitatively.  It  is, 
however,  doubtful  that  the  effects  produced  bv  stimu- 
lation of  the  sensorimotor  area  could  have  been 
mediated  by  the  pathways  proposed  by  the  Norwegian 
workers  since,  if  this  were  so,  one  could  have  expected 
indiscriminate  effects  from  stimulation  of  anv  cortical 
region  in  either  hemisphere,  which  apparentk  did 
not  occur. 

Activation  of  Brain  Stem  Reluiilar  Formation  by 
Sensory  Somatic  Discharges 

In  1949,  Moruzzi  &  Magoun  (179)  reported  that 
electrical  stimulation  of  the  medial  portions  of  the 
medulla,  of  the  pontine  and  midbrain  tegmentum, 
and  of  the  dorsal  hypothalamus  and  subthalainus 
produces  generalized  changes  in  the  EEG  which 
appear  identical  with  those  which  result  when  the 
animal  is  aroused  from  sleep  or  alerted  to  attention. 
They  suggested,  therefore,  that  the  central  core  of 
the  brain  stem  represents  an  ascending  activating 
system,  the  activity  of  the  system  being  essential  for 
wakefulness,  the  depression  of  this  activity  producing 
normal  sleep  or  somnolence.  A  great  deal  of  effort 


has  been  expended  in  recent  years,  particularly  by 
the  research  group  of  Magoun,  by  the  group  of 
Moruzzi  in  Italy  and  by  Bremer  and  his  colleagues,  to 
substantiate  this  concept — which  was  suggested  by  the 
early  work  of  Bremer  (30,  31) — and  to  determine  the 
functional  organization  of  the  reticular  ascending 
system  and  the  sources  of  its  inflow. 

Just  to  what  extent  and  in  which  sense  one  can 
consider  the  reticular  activating  system  as  a  functional 
unit  (it  is  certainly  not  a  unit  morphologically)  is  at 
present  still  conjectural.  Basic  as  this  question  may  be 
for  considerations  of  sensations  in  general,  we  shall  not 
discuss  it  further  since  the  present  status  of  the  prob- 
lem is  presented  in  detail  in  Chapter  XLII  of  this 
work.  However,  we  shall  consider  briefly  the  evidence 
regarding  the  activation  of  the  brain  stem  by  somatic 
sensory  afferents  since  the  available  evidence  suggests, 
we  ijelieve,  a  departure  from  conclusions  usually 
reached  on  this  subject. 

Little  is  known  about  actisation  of  brain-stem 
groupings  by  natural  tactile  stimuli.  One  may  pre- 
sume, however,  that  activity  aroused  by  electrical 
stimulation  of  a  nerve  (it  was  usually  the  sciatic  nerve 
which  was  stimulated)  reflects,  at  least  partially, 
activity  aroused  by  tactile  stimulation  as  well. 

In  a  series  of  papers  the  C^alifornia  workers  (72-74, 
229,  230)  concluded  that,  in  addition  to  the  medial 
lemniscal  system,  there  exists  in  the  brainstem  a 
medially  located,  multisynaptic  path,  conducting 
centripetally,  which  is  fed  by  collaterals  arising  from 
virtualh  the  entire  length  of  the  medial  lemniscus. 
They  felt  that  this  medial  system  must  be  composed 
of  a  multisynaptic  chain  of  neurons  since  in  compari- 
son with  the  potentials  in  the  lemniscal  system  those 
evoked  in  the  reticular  formation  displayed  much 
longer  latencies  and  longer  recovery  time  and  were 
more  sensitive  to  anesthetics.  It  is  likely  that  the 
California  workers  understand  by  the  medial  lemnis- 
cal system,  not  only  the  system  which  we  have  defined 
under  that  term  but  also  the  spinothalamic  tract. 
Nevertheless,  all  their  data  pertaining  to  the  classical 
pathways  seem  to  refer  to  the  medial  lemniscal  system, 
as  we  understand  it,  and  there  is  little  doubt  that 
collaterals  of  the  classical  medial  lemniscal  pathway 
are  believed  to  activate  the  reticular  ascending  system. 

While  it  is,  of  course,  possible  that  the  reticular 
potentials  are  indeed  of  medial  lemniscal  origin,  it 
appears  more  likely  that  they  are  evoked,  at  least 
predominantly,  through  activation  of  the  antero- 
lateral columns  in  the  spinal  cord  rather  than  of  the 
medial  lemniscus.  There  are  several  reasons  for  this 
belief.  First  of  all  there  is  little  anatomical  evidence 


42-2 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


that  the  fibers  of  the  medial  lemniscus  emit  any  sub- 
stantial number  of  collaterals.  Ramon  y  Cajal  (201) 
was  emphatic  in  this  respect  and  believed  that  only  a 
few  collaterals  were  given  off  to  the  region  of  the  red 
nucleus  and  a  somewhat  larger  number  to  the  pretec- 
tal region.  He  seems  to  have  been  hesitant  in  respect 
to  the  medial  lemniscal  contribution  to  the  mamillary 
peduncle.  Other  workers  occasionally  described  some 
collaterals  to  other  regions  as  well  but  there  seems  to 
be  no  convincing  evidence  that  the  medial  lemniscus 
gives  off  any  substantial  number  of  collaterals  below 
the  midbrain.  Matzke  (170)  recently  stressed  that  the 
medial  lemniscus  does  not  decrease  perceptibly  in 
size  from  its  origin  to  its  termination. 

The  situation  is  different  for  the  fibers  arising  in  the 
posterior  horns  and  ascending  in  the  anterolateral 
column.  We  have  considered  thus  far  exclusively  one 
component  of  the  group,  the  spinothalamic  tract, 
since  it  reaches  farthest  orally.  However,  it  is  well 
known  that  there  are  other  fibers  accompanying  the 
spinothalamic  tract  which  terminate  below  the  thal- 
amus and  it  is  likely  that  at  least  some  of  them  conduct 
impulses  evoked  by  cutaneous  stimuli.  The  fact  that 
after  anterolateral  cordotomy  only  a  small  fraction  of 
the  degenerating  fibers  reach  the  thalamus  led,  as 
was  discussed  earlier,  to  doubts  as  to  the  existence  of 
the  spinothalamic  tract  in  some  mammals.  It  appears 
then  that  collaterals  from  the  spinothalamic  tract  or 
other  anterolateral  column  fibers  terminating  at 
lower  levels  could  provide  obvious  pathways  for 
relaying  sensory-somatic  activity  to  the  brain-stem 
structures  without  any  strain  on  the  known  anatomical 
facts  concerning  the  medial  lemniscus. 

Second,  it  has  been  shown  that  the  brainstem 
potentials  evoked  by  stimulation  of  the  sciatic  nerve 
occur  bilaterally  (230).  This  observation  is  quite  in 
harmony  with  the  presumed  existence  of  the  ipsi-  and 
contralateral  tracts  in  the  anterolateral  columns.  On 
the  other  hand,  the  occurrence  of  such  ipsilateral  po- 
tentials would  be  rather  puzzling  if  they  were  medi- 
ated by  the  lemniscal  system  since,  despite  some 
protestations  to  the  contrary,  there  is,  we  believe,  no 
evidence  for  existence  of  an  ipsilateral  lemniscus  aris- 
ing in  the  posterior  column  nuclei. 

Third,  it  has  been  shown  that  the  potentials  evoked 
in  the  ascending  reticular  system  by  stimulation  of  the 
sciatic  nerve  possess  substantially  longer  latencies  than 
do  responses  recorded  at  virtually  any  level  of  the 
lemniscal  system  (73).  It  is  not  at  all  clear  why  this 
should  be  so  if  a  collateral  inflow  for  the  ascending 
activating  system  were  indeed  available  almost  at  all 
levels  from   the  medial   lemniscus.    If,   on   the  other 


hand,  these  potentials  were  evoked  by  mediation  of 
predominantly  small  fibers  of  the  anterolateral  col- 
umns their  long  latencies  could  be  readily  understood 
even  if  the  number  of  intercalated  synaptic  regions 
was  quite  small.  That  some  potentials  in  the  brain 
stem  are,  as  could  be  expected,  relayed  through  the 
anterolateral  column  has  been  shown  recently  by 
Collins  &  O'Leary  (54).  These  workers  studied  a 
small  region  in  the  midbrain  which  was  activated 
when  smaller  fibers  (gamma  and  delta  groups)  of  the 
radial  or  sciatic  nerves  were  excited.  They  could  show 
that  the  midbrain  potentials  survived  (in  contrast  to 
the  potential  evoked  in  the  ventrobasal  complex)  a 
destruction  of  the  homolateral  posterior  column 
(homolateral  to  the  stimulated  nerve)  but  were 
abolished  (again  in  contrast  to  the  potentials  evoked 
in  the  ventrobasal  complex)  when  the  contralateral 
anterolateral  column  was  destroyed.  It  may  be  em- 
phasized that  the  midbrain  potentials  displayed  much 
longer  latencies  than  did  the  potentials  evoked  in  the 
medial  lemniscal  system,  and  in  contrast  to  the  latter 
were  sensitive  to  anesthetic  agents. 

In  summary,  it  appears  that  the  sources  of  the 
sensory  somatic  inflow  which  activate  the  various 
brain  stem  structures  are  not  yet  established  une- 
quivocally. The  evidence  at  hand  seems  to  imply  that 
the  potentials  recorded  by  the  California  workers  in 
the  reticular  activating  system  relay  mainly  or  solely 
through  the  anterolateral  columns  and  the  tracts  aris- 
ing from  them  rather  than  through  the  posterior 
column  and  the  medial  lemniscal  system.  However, 
these  are  indirect  conclusions  and  it  would  \x  desir- 
al^le  to  test  them  experimentally. 

Cortical  Fields   Olliir    Than   the  Primary   Receiving  Area 
Which  Are  Activated  by  Tactile  Stimuli 

We  ha\e  thus  far  proceeded  on  the  assumption 
that,  among  all  the  discharges  in  the  central  nervous 
system  which  are  provoked  by  cutaneous  and  deep 
stimuli,  only  tho.se  which  occur  in  the  medial  lem- 
niscal or  spinothalamic  systems  are  relevant  for  the 
arousal  of  tactile  and  kinesthetic  sensations.  Clinical 
evidence  suggests  that  this  is  likely  to  be  so  for  all  the 
synaptic  regions  below  the  cortical  le\el.  For  the  cor- 
tex itself  the  situation  is  less  clear  mainly  because  one 
is  uncertain  as  to  the  extent  of  the  cortical  fields  which 
are  directly  activated  by  the  subcortical  components 
of  the  classical  systems.  There  is,  of  course,  no  doubt 
that  the  postcentral  region  in  primates  or  its  homo- 
logue  in  other  mammals  (which  can  be  defined  as  the 
projection   field   of  the   ventrobasal   complex   of  the 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


423 


thalamus)  is  the  chief  cortical  representative  of  the 
medial  lemniscal  system.  As  such  it  must  be  in  some 
way  critically  involved  in  the  elaboration  of  tactile 
and  kinesthetic  sensations,  and  we  have  already  con- 
sidered some  functional  properties  of  this  field.  Never- 
theless, there  is  evidence  available  that  this  region  is 
not  the  e.xclusive  recipient  of  all  the  discharges  trans- 
mitted through  the  medial  lemniscal  and  spino- 
thalamic relays.  While  the  evidence  to  this  effect  is 
fragmentary  and  much  more  work  is  needed  to  eluci- 
date the  details  the  general  picture  which  emerges 
seems  to  be  as  follows.  The  postcentral  family  of  fields 
(areas  i  to  3  of  primates)  appears  to  represent  a  focal 
region  for  tactile  and  kinesthetic  activity.  This 
activity  is  undoubtedly  based  primarily  on  the  inflow 
from  the  ventrobasal  complex.  .Surrounding  this  core 
region  is  a  belt  of  cortical  fields  which  receive,  apart 
from  any  connections  they  may  have  with  the  post- 
central region  itself,  some  sensory  somatic  inflow 
directly  from  the  thalamus.  It  is  convenient  to  con- 
sider the  evidence  under  several  headings. 

.^NATOMic^L  EVIDENCE.  Most  of  the  cclls  of  the  ventro- 
basal complex  are  definitely  known  to  project  exclu- 
sively upon  the  postcentral  region,  and  it  seems  likely 
that  all  do  so.  Assuming  this  to  be  true,  it  follows  that 
if  impulses  evoked  by  tactile  stimuli  are  transmitted 
directly  from  the  thalamus  to  some  fields  surrounding 
the  postcentral  region,  they  must  be  relayed  through 
thalamic  nuclei  other  than  the  ventrobasal  complex. 
In  fact  evidence  is  available  that  a  thalamic  element 
intercalated  between  the  ventrobasal  complex  and  the 
medial  geniculate  body  projects  upon  the  second 
somatic  field  (see  p.  418).  It  seems  probable  further 
that  this  element  may  be  a  terminal  station  for  some 
spinothalamic  fibers.  Whether  other  thalamic  ele- 
ments of  the  ventral  or  posterior  nuclear  groups  which 
partly  surround  the  ventrobasal  complex  receive  some 
medial  lemniscal  or  spinothalamic  fibers  is  not  clear. 
It  is  tempting  to  speculate  that  for  some  this  may  be 
true  since,  if  this  were  so,  a  number  of  electrophysio- 
logical observations  would  be  readily  understandable. 

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL  EVIDENCE.  It  has  been  known 
since  the  early  days  of  the  evoked  potential  technique 
that  the  extent  of  the  cortical  areas  activated  by  sen- 
sory somatic  stimuli  may  vary  with  the  anesthetic 
state.  In  deeply  pentobarbitalized  animals  it  is  usually 
the  classical  first  and  second  somatic  fields  which  are 
activated  by  natural  tactile  stimuli.  Under  different 
anesthetic  conditions,  with  no  anesthesia  at  all,  or 
when  such  drugs  as  chloralose  are  used,  potentials 


may  appear  in  other  regions  as  well,  sometimes  only 
when  nerve  volleys  are  used  as  stimuli  (i,  8,  9,  14,  38, 
39,  84,  92,  142,  165,  167,  276).  [Other  references  are 
given  by  Buser  (37).] 

In  the  cat,  which  was  most  extensively  studied, 
such  additional  regions  in  which  potentials  evoked  by 
sensory  somatic  stimuli  are  likely  to  appear  most 
consistently  are  the  precentral  homologue,  the  an- 
terior portion  of  the  lateral  gyrus  and  the  suprasylvian 
gyrus.  It  is  known  that  a  removal  of  the  first  somatic 
area  (38,  278)  does  not  abolish  e\oked  potentials  in 
the  second  .somatic  field.  Recently  it  has  been  shown 
by  Albe-Fessard  &  Rougeul  (8)  that  responses  in  the 
lateral  and  the  suprasylvian  gyri  are  likewise  not 
abolished  by  such  a  removal.  Similar  evidence  has 
been  offered  (84,  142,  165)  in  respect  to  potentials 
evoked  in  the  precentral  region  of  the  monkey  by 
stimulation  of  the  afferent  nerves. 

The  fact  that  such  potentials  tend  to  appear  under 
special  conditions  of  stimulation  or  when  the  excita- 
bility of  neurons  is  enhanced  by  drugs  hardly  di- 
minishes the  significance  of  the  phenomenon  if 
transcortical  spread  from  the  postcentral  area  can  be 
excluded.  Since  this  was  done  for  several  regions  sur- 
rounding this  area  the  conclusion  seems  warranted 
that,  under  certain  conditions  at  least,  some  sensory 
somatic  discharges  may  relay  to  other  regions  of  the 
neocortex  without  mediation  of  the  primary  receiving 
field. 

EXPERi.MENT.\L  psvcHOLOGic.vL  EVIDENCE.  The  elec- 
trophysiological evidence  suggests  then  rather  strongly 
that  not  only  the  postcentral  region  but  also  a  belt  of 
fields  around  it  may  be  of  significance  for  the  capacity 
of  the  animal  to  appreciate  and  handle  sensory  somatic 
information.  Nevertheless,  it  is  reasonable  to  expect 
somatic  area  I  to  be  the  central  region  of  the  somes- 
thetic  system.  The  problem  is  to  determine  the  exact 
role  played  by  the  different  fields  in  the  somesthetic 
capacity  of  the  animal  since  it  is  obvious  that  these 
fields  cannot  by  any  means  be  functionally  equivalent. 
Unfortunately  only  a  small  number  of  studies  is  avail- 
able on  the  ability  of  an  animal  to  perform  somesthe- 
tic discrimination  tasks  after  ablations  of  the  post- 
central field  or  other  cortical  regions.  A  systematic 
analysis  has  not  proceeded  very  far  perhaps  because 
of  the  confusion  which  was  created  by  the  finding  that 
simple  somesthetic  discriminations  are  still  possible  or 
can  be  relearned  after  removal  of  the  first  somatic 
field.  The  studies  of  Ruch  &  Fulton  (210),  who  re- 
viewed the  older  literature  on  the  subject,  although 
severely  handicapped  by  lack  of  anatomical  controls, 


424 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


were  suggestive  that  this  might  be  so  in  primates. 
Recent  findings  in  rat  (283,  285,  286),  cat  (284)  and 
dog  (10,  11)  provide  acceptable  evidence  for  this 
statement.  Ablations  of  somatic  areas  I  and  II  to- 
gether led  to  findings  which  may  but  need  not  be 
discordant.  In  the  rat  (283,  286)  discrimination  of 
roughness  can  apparently  be  relearned  and  form 
discrimination  need  not  be  lost  after  such  ablations. 
Likewise  form  can  be  discriminated  satisfactorily  by 
the  monkey  with  some  retraining  (143),  even  though 
a  severe  loss  of  tactile  acuity  is  evident  when  placing 
reactions  or  grasp  reflexes  are  tested.  On  the  other 
hand,  in  the  cat  (284)  and  dog  (i  i)  a  persistent  in- 
ability of  the  animal  to  discriminate  somesthetic 
stimuli  has  been  reported  after  such  combined  re- 
movals. The  findings  in  the  cat  can  be  dismissed  since 
they  are  based  on  studies  on  one  animal  only.  The 
findings  of  Allen  on  the  dog  require  some  comment. 
It  appears  from  his  description  that  the  actual  cortical 
removals  exceeded  considerably  the  anatomical  limits 
of  SI  and  SII.  This  might  but  probably  does  not 
account  for  Allen's  divergent  findings.  What  seems  to 
be  a  more  probable  explanation  of  his  results  is  that — 
in  contrast  to  all  other  obsersers  here  considered  who 
used  a  pair  of  discriminanda  differing  only  in  weight, 
roughness  or  form — Allen  alone  employed  a  differ- 
ential conditioning  technique  to  test  the  somesthetic 
defects  of  his  animals.  The  response  of  the  animal 
consisted  of  lifting  his  foreleg  to  a  positive  stiinulus 
and  not  lifting  it  to  a  negative  one.  The  positive  and 
negative  stimuli  were  respectively  a  light  stroking  of 
the  iwck  with  the  grain  and  against  the  grain  or  a 
light  stroking  with  the  grain  once  a  second  (positive) 
and  three  times  a  second  (negatixe).  The  chief  defect 
observed  was  the  inability  of  the  operated  animals  to 
withold  the  foreleg  response  when  negative  stimuli 
were  applied.  The  response  to  positive  stimuli  was 
retained  without  impairment.  In  short,  after  the  oper- 
ations the  animals  tended  to  lift  the  leg  to  both  the 
negative  and  po.sitive  stimuli  and  could  not  be  re- 
trained to  make  the  differentiation.  Since  the  task  .set 
by  Allen  for  his  animals  was  quite  difi"erent  and  prob- 
ably more  subtle  than  those  set  by  the  other  observers, 
the  different  results  need  not  imply  contradictory 
findings. 

Even  though  older  anatomical  and  physiological 
views  have  often  held  that  the  precentral  and  post- 
central regions  somehow  form  a  functional  unit  of  a 
higher  order,  it  is  only  recently  that  the  effects  of 
postcentral  and  precentral  ablation  upon  the  somes- 
thetic capacity  of  the  monkey  have  been  tested  with 
modern  techniques.  As  already  mentioned,  Kruger  & 


Porter  C'43)  found  no  permanent  deficits  in  somes- 
thetic form  discrimination  after  remo\al  of  the  somatic 
areas  I  and  II  in  their  monkeys  even  though  a  severe 
tactile  impairment  of  the  limbs  could  be  reasonably 
inferred  to  be  present.  Likewise,  there  was  a  perfect 
retention  of  the  learned  habit  if  the  precentral  gyrus 
alone  was  removed  despite  the  .severe  motor  deficits 
in  the  limbs.  However,  if  both  these  regions  were  re- 
moved jointK  on  one  side  the  two  animals  tested 
could  not  be  trained  to  discriminate  with  the  contra- 
lateral hand  a  three  dimensional  figure  '  L'  from  its 
in\ersion.  However,  a  \isual  discrimination  task  could 
be  carried  out  utilizing  that  limb.  Since  this  work  re- 
ports permanent  deficits,  further  studies  along  these 
lines  are  urgently  needed.  The  reported  lesions  ex- 
tended farther  caudally  than  the  anatomical  limits  of 
areas  i  to  3.  It  remains  to  be  determined  whether  an 
inclusion  of  at  least  a  part  of  the  parietal  region  is 
necessary  to  produce  permanent  discrimination  def- 
icits. 

The  knowledge  that  some  tactile  responses  from  the 
ipsilateral  side  of  the  body  reach  the  ipsilateral  cortex 
(although  we  believe  this  is  not  true  for  the  first 
somatic  field)  could  suggest  that  learning;  of  at  least 
simple  somesthetic  discriminations  takes  place  simul- 
taneously in  both  hemispheres.  Stamm  &  Sperry's 
(22B)  results  are,  therefore,  somewhat  surprising.  In 
their  cats  the  discrimination  of  form,  softness  and 
roughness  performed  with  one  paw  had  to  be  com- 
pletely relearned  only  if  the  corpus  callosum  was 
sectioned.  Clearly  more  data  are  needed  to  substanti- 
ate and  further  elucidate  this  problem. 

The  last  set  of  available  data  we  wish  to  consider 
pertains  to  findings  after  lesions  of  the  parietal  cortex. 
It  was  already  suggested  b\  the  older  work  (21 1-2 13) 
that  parietal  lesions  may  produce  some  deficits  in  the 
capacity  to  discriminate  somesthetic  cues  even  though 
the  animals  could  usually  relearn  the  tasks.  Such 
deficits  after  ablations  of  the  parietal  cortex  which 
spared  the  postcentral  region  itself  were  definitely 
established  by  Blum  (23),  Blum  et  al.  (24)  and  Pribram 
&  Barry  (199).  Blum  et  al.  (24)  made  the  tentative 
suggestion  that  processes  which  determine  the  somes- 
thetic discrimination  capacity  of  the  animal  take  place 
outside  the  postcentral  region  itself  and  specifically  in 
the  parietal  region.  The  available  findings  hardly  per- 
mit an  evaluation  of  this  suggestion.  The  data  at  hand 
are  as  yet  too  few  in  number,  too  limited  in  scope  and 
not  sufficicntU  systematic  with  respect  to  .some  corti- 
cal fields  which  are  probably  or  possibly  relevant. 
Some  important  observations,  therefore,  are  subject 
to   different   interpretations.    For   example,    the   im- 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


425 


portant  finding  of  all  workers  that  the  removal  of  the 
postcentral  region  does  not  necessarily  lead  to  any 
early  or  permanent  deficits  in  somesthetic  discrimina- 
tions indicates  strictly  only  that  the  postcentral  region 
is  not  the  sole  cortical  recipient  and  distributor  of  all 
the  corticopetal  tactile  activity.  This,  of  course,  is  also 
apparent  from  the  electrophysiological  considerations. 
Whether,  however,  such  findings  indicate  in  any  way 
that  the  postcentral  cortex  is  not  relevant  for  processes 
determining  the  somesthetic  discrimination  capacity 
of  the  animal  is  another  question.  The  anatomical  and 
electrophysiological  evidence  leaves  hardly  any  douijt 
that  it  must  be  relevant.  The  experimental  psycho- 
physical data  are  indicative  that  the  postcentral  region 
need  not  always  be  essential.  It  seems  reasonable  to 
consider  the  possibility  that  the  answer  may  lie  in  the 
nature  of  the  somesthetic  task  which  the  animal  is 
trained  to  perform.  If  the  somesthetic  cues  used  in  the 
experiments  differed  crudely  from  each  other  (and 
certainly  most  of  them  did  so),  it  is  possible  that  any 
somatic  sensory  inflow  which  reaches  the  cortex  after 
removal  of  the  postcentral  region  still  contains  enough 
information  to  enable  the  animal  to  perform  the  task. 
It  seems  probable  that  the  unique  role  of  the  first 
somatic  field  will  become  apparent  if  the  animal  is 
asked  to  perform  a  task  requiring  the  highly  detailed 
and  complex  information  which,  as  it  appears,  is 
available  only  to  the  primary  receiving  field.  If  this 
were  so  the  postcentral  cortex  would  be  necessary  for 
any  tactile  or  kinesthetic  discrimination  task  of  suffi- 
cient complexity.  Whether  it  alone  is  ever  sufficient 
for  learning  of  such  a  discrimination  is  yet  10  be  de- 
termined. The  chances  are  that  the  answer  to  this 
question  will  depend  on  what  the  animal  is  asked  to 
do  with  the  information  it  has  available. 


CONCLUDING    REM.ARKS 

It  may  be  useful  to  discuss  at  the  conclusion  of  this 
chapter  experimental  work  which  appears  necessary 
for  the  clarification  of  some  ideas  regarding  tactile  and 
kinesthetic  sensations. 

In  the  section  dealing  with  the  neural  events  in  the 
peripheral  fibers  it  has  been  pointed  out  that  a  re\ival 
in  some  form  of  the  basic  concepts  of  Head  may  be- 
come advisable.  What  is  well  established  is  that 
specific  tactile  receptors  exist;  what  can  be  deduced 
from  some  observations  but  what  is  bv  no  means  vet 
demonstrated  is  that  generalized  receptors,  presum- 
ably responding  to  all  modes  of  cutaneous  stimulation, 
may  exist  as  well.  It  seems  futile  to  denv  or  ignore 


the  convincing  evidence  regarding  the  specificity  of 
some  tactile  receptors;  it  is  probably  too  much  to 
expect,  on  the  other  hand,  that  all  experimental  find- 
ings will  yet  become  understandable  within  the  frame- 
work of  von  Frey's  concepts.  It  seems  probable  that 
the  clouds  over  the  classic  concepts  are  real  and  that 
further  work  will  establish  the  existence  of  generalized 
(in  addition  to  the  specific)  receptors  which  will 
probably  reopen  the  question  of  the  existence  of 
epicritic  and  protopathic  sensibility. 

Assuming  that  this  development  will  take  place  the 
problem  will  be  to  determine  to  what  extent  the 
medial  lemniscal  and  the  spinothalamic  systems  are, 
respectively,  activated  by  these  two  types  of  receptors. 
As  far  as  the  tactile  activity  is  concerned  there  is 
hardly  any  doubt  that  specific  receptors  activate  the 
medial  lemniscal  system.  The  available  evidence  indi- 
cates that  this  system  could  represent  the  tactile  (and 
kinesthetic)  epicritic  system.  The  fundamental  ques- 
tion as  to  whether  this  system  can  be  activated  as  well 
by  nociceptive  and  thermal  stimuli  must  remain  un- 
answered at  the  moment.  In  anesthetized  animals 
only  mechanical  stimuli  activate  the  medial  lemniscal 
system.  It  is,  however,  not  known  whether  this  repre- 
sents the  true  state  of  affairs  or  whether  such  findings 
are  caused  by  anesthesia.  The  spinothalamic  system 
could  be  the  obvious  representative  of  the  protopathic 
system  if  the  latter  should  exist.  What  little  is  known 
about  its  tactile  activity  is  compatible  with  the  idea 
that  generalized  receptors  activate  it.  It  is  this  system 
which — in  sharp  contrast  to  the  medial  lemni.scal 
system — seems  to  distriijute,  together  with  other  tracts 
arising  in  the  posterior  horns  the  sensory  somatic 
activity  throughout  the  brain  stem.  An  extensi\'e  inter- 
locking between  the  medial  lemniscal  and  the  spino- 
thalamic systems  occurs  in  two  places.  The  first  is  the 
synaptic  region  of  the  posterior  horns  where  the 
lemniscal  activity  plays  upon  the  cells  of  origin  of  the 
spinothalamic  system;  the  .second  is  the  ventrobasal 
complex  of  the  thalamus  where  the  spinothalamic 
activity  in  turn  interacts  with  the  medial  lemniscal 
system.  However,  nothing  at  all  is  known  as  to  the 
meaning  of  these  interactions.  From  clinical  studies  it 
is  clear  that  sensations  of  pain,  temperature  and 
tickle,  and  those  accompanying  sexual  excitement  de- 
pend upon  the  integrity  of  the  spinothalamic  .system. 
Although  this  knowledge  has  been  gained  on  large 
human  material,  very  little  is  known  for  certain  about 
the  functional  organization  of  the  system,  not  even 
whether  or  not  its  fibers  are  modality  specific.  It  is 
obvious  that  a  number  of  basic  problems  of  sensation 
could   be  profitably  explored  in  man  in  connection 


426 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


with  the  frequently  performed  pain-relieving  opera- 
tions. Unfortunately  such  problems  seem  to  be  seldom 
of  interest  to  clinical  observers. 

In  regard  to  kinesthetic  sensations  considerable 
progress  has  been  achieved  in  recent  years.  First  of 
all,  it  is  now  apparent  that  the  sense  of  position  and  of 
movements  of  the  joints  depends  solely  on  the  appro- 
priate receptors  in  the  joints  themselves.  There  is  no 
need  to  invoke  a  mysterious '  muscle'  sense  to  explain 
kinesthetic  sensations,  and  to  do  so  runs  contrary  to  all 
the  known  facts  concerning  the  muscle  stretch  re- 
ceptors. 

A  second  point  is  that  kinesthetic  activity  is  relayed 
in  the  medial  lemniscal  system,  as  could  have  been 
expected  from  clinical  experiences  in  man.  Cells  con- 
cerned with  kinesthetic  activity  are  intermingled  at 
each  synaptic  level  with  the  cells  concerned  with  the 
activity  evoked  by  tactile  receptors,  and  the  two 
groups  are  arranged  in  one  common  representation 


pattern.  The  individual  cells  retain,  as  far  as  is  known, 
their  modality  specificity  at  least  to  the  first  stage  of 
cortical  activation. 

A  third  fact  of  great  interest  is  that  receptors  from 
bones,  periosteum,  deep  fascia  and  sheaths  of  tendons 
activate  the  medial  lemniscal  system  in  exactly  the 
same  fashion  as  do  the  tactile  skin  and  joint  receptors. 
Whether  kinesthetic  and  deep  stimuli  activate  also  the 
spinothalamic  system  is  unknown. 

At  present  the  conclusion  must  be  that  touch,  pres- 
sure, kinesthesis  and  deep  sensibility  are  all  very 
closely  related.  Yet  this  is  not  at  all  apparent  from 
introspective  observations,  at  least  not  for  touch  and 
kinesthesis.  It  seems  likely  that  more  will  have  to  be 
known  about  cortical  handling  of  the  neural  activity 
evoked  \)y  sensory  stimuli  before  one  can  approach 
such  problems  on  other  than  a  purely  speculative 
basis. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Adev,  VV.  R.,  R.  Porter  .•^nd  I.  D.  Carter.  Brain  77:  24. 

325.  1954- 

2.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Phsyiol.  6i ;  49,  1926.  25. 

3.  .Adrian,    E.    D.    Proc.    Rov.   Soc.    London.    Ser.    B    log:    i,  26. 
1932. 

4.  ."Adrian,    E.    D.,   McK.    C.'Kttell   .■\nd    H.    Ho.^gl.iiND.  27. 
J.  Physiol.  72:  377,  1931. 

5.  Adrian,    E.   D.    and   K.    Umrath.   J.    Physiol .    68:    139, 

1929.  28. 

6.  Adrian,   E.  D.  and   Y.  Zotterman.  J.    Physiol.  61  ;  464,            29. 
1926. 

7.  Albe-Fessard,  D.  J.  physiol..  Pans  49:  522,  1957.                            30. 

8.  Albe-Fessard,  D.    and    A.    Rougeul.   J.   physiol.,    Paris            31. 
47:69,  1955.  32- 

9.  Albe-Fessard,  D.    and    A.    Rougeul.   J.  physiol.,    Paris            33. 

48;  370.  1956. 

10.  Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  147:  454,  1946.  34. 

11.  Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  151 :  325,  1947. 

12.  Alvarez-Buylla,    R.    and   R.    De    .\rellano.    Am.   J.  35. 
Physiol.  172:237,  1953. 

13.  Amassian,   V.    E.    Eleclrocnrephalog.   &    Clin.    .A'europhysiol .  36. 

5:4'5.  igSS-  37- 

14.  Amassian,  V.  E.  J.  .\europhysiol.  17:  39,  1954.  38. 

15.  Amassi.\n,  V.  E.  AND  J.  L.  DeVito.  Coll.  Intern,  centre  not. 
recherche  Sci.  67:  353,  1957.  39. 

16.  .Andrew,  B.  L.  J.  Physiol.  123:  241,  1954. 

17.  .Andrew,  B.  L.  J.  Physiol.  126:  507,  1954.  40. 

18.  .Andrew,  B.  L.  and  E.  Dodt.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  28:  41. 
287,  1953. 

19.  AsTROM,  K.  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  Suppl.   106,  29:  209,  42. 

'953- 

20.  Berry,  C.  M.,  R.  C.  Karl  and  J.  C.  Hinsey.  J.  Nemo-  43. 

physiol.  13:  149,  1950.  44. 

21.  Bishop,  G.  H.  Physiol.  Rev.  26:  77,  1946.  45. 

22.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  P.  Heinbecker.  .-im.  J.  Physiol.   114: 

179.  '935-  46- 

23.  Blum,  J.  S.  Comp.  Psychol,  .\lonogr.  20:  219,  1931. 


Blum,  J.  S.,  K.  L.  Chow  and  K.  H.  Pribram.  J.  Comp. 

.Neurol.  93:  53,  1950. 

BoHM,  E.  Acta  physiol.  .scandinav.  ?>\ipi>\.  106,  29:  106,  1953. 

BoHM,  E.  AND  I.  Petersen,  .icta  physiol.  scandinav.  Suppl. 

106,  29:  138,  1953. 

BoK,  S.  T.   In :  W.  von  MoUendorff,  Handbuch  der  mikro- 

skopischen  Anatomic  des  .Menschen.   Berlin;   Springer,    1928, 

vol.  4,  p.  478. 

Boyd,  I.  .\.  J.  Physiol.  124:476,  1954. 

Boyd,  I.  .\.  and  T.  D.  M.  Roberts.  J.  Physiol.   122:   38, 

1953- 

Bremer,  F.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  118:  1235,  1935. 
Bremer,  F.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  1 18:  1241,  1935. 
Brodal,  a.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  57:  292,  1947. 
Brodal,  .\.  and  B.  R.  Kaad.\.  J.  .Xeurophysiol.   16:  567, 

'953- 

Brodal,  .\.,  T.  .Szabo  and  .A.  Torvik.  J.  Comp.  .Keurol. 

106:  527,  1956. 

Brodal,  A.   and  F.   Walberg.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat. 

68:  755.  '952- 

BiJRGi,  S.  Arch.  Psychiat.  194:  67,  1955. 

Buser,  p.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  49:  589,  1957. 

BusER,  P.  and  p.  Borenstein.  J.  physiol.,  Paris  48:  419, 

1956- 

Buser,   P.    and   G.    Heinze.   J.  physiol..   Pans  46:   284, 

'954- 

Carpenter,  M.  B.  J.  .inat.  91  :  82,  1957. 

Catalano,  J.  V.  AND  G.  Lamarche.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  189: 

141.  1957- 

Cattell,  McK.  and  H.  Hoagland.  J.  Physwl.  72:  392, 

1931- 

Chang,  H.-T.  and  T.  C.  Ruch.  J.  Anat.  81  :  140,  1947. 

Chang,  H.-T.  and  T.  C.  Ruch.  J.  Anat.  81 :  150,  1947. 

Chang,  H.-T.,  C.  N.  Woolsey,  L.  VV.  Jarcho  and  E. 

Hennem.'^n.  Fed.  Proc.  6:  89,  1947. 

Clark,  D.,  J.  Hughes  .\nd  H.  S.  G.\sser.  .^m.  J.  Physiol. 

114:  69,  1935. 


TOUCH    AND    KINETHESIS 


47.  Clark,  W.  E.  Le  Gros.  J.  Anat.  71:  7,  1936. 

48.  Clark,  \V.  E.  Le  Gros.  J.  Ment.  Sc.  82:  99,  1936. 

49.  Clark,  W.  E.  Le  Gros  and  R.  H.  Boggon.  Phil.  Trans. 
6224:313,  1935. 

50.  Clark,  W.  E.  Le  Gros  and  T.  P.  S.  Powell.  Proc.  Roy. 
Soc,  London,  ser.  B  141 :  467,  1953. 

51.  Cohen,  S.  M.  J.  JVeurophysiol.  18:  33,  1955. 

52.  Cohen,  S.   M.   and  H.   Grundfest.  J.  .Neurophysud .    17: 

193.  1954- 

53.  Collier,  J.  and  E.  F.  Buzzard.  Brain  26:  559,  1903. 

54.  Collins,  W.  F.  and  J.  L.  O'Leary.  Electrocncephalog.  & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.  6:  619,  1954. 

55.  Crawford,  A.  S.  and  R.  S.  Knighton.  J.  Nemosurg. 
10:  113,  1953. 

56.  Dejerine,  J.  .and  Mme.  J.  Dejerine.  Compl.  rend.  Soc.  de 
biol.  2  :  285,   1895. 

57.  D'Errico,  a.  J.  .Xeurosurg.  7:  294,  1950. 

58.  Drake,  C.  G.   and  K.  G.  McKenzie.  J.  .\'eurosurg.    10: 

457.  >953- 

59.  DussER  de  Barenne,  J.  G.  .and  O.  Sager.  ^eitsch.  ges. 
Neurol.  Psychiat.  133;  231,  1931. 

60.  Dusser  de  Barenne,  J.  G.  and  O.  Sager.  Arch,  .\eurol.  & 
Psychiat.  38:913,  1937. 

61.  Edinger,  L.  Anal.  Anz.  4:  121,  1889. 

62.  Eldred,  E.,  R.  Granit  and  P.  A.  Merton.  J.  Physiol.  122: 

498,  1953- 

63.  Eldred,   E.   and   K.-E.   H.agb.arth.  J.  \europhysiol.    17: 

59.  ■954- 

64.  Falconer,  M.  .\.  J.  .\eurol.  Neurosurg.  S  Psychiat.  12  :  297, 

■949- 

65.  Ferraro,  .\.  AND  S.  E.  Barrera.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat. 
33:  262,  1934. 

66.  Ferraro,  A.,  and  S.   E.  Barrera.  J.  Conip.  .Xeurol.  62 : 

507,  1935- 

67.  Ferraro,  A.   and  S.   E.  Barrera.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  64: 

313.  '936- 

68.  Fitzgerald,  O.  J.  Physiol.  98:  163,  1940. 

6g.  Flechsio,  P.  and  O.  Hosel.  Neurol.  Cenlralbl.  9:  417, 
1890. 

70.  Foerster,  O.  In:  Handbuch  der  .Xeurologie,  edited  by  O. 
Bumke  and  O.  Foerster.  Berlin:  Springer,  1936,  vol.  5, 
p.  I. 

71.  Foerster,  O.  and  O.  G.agel.  ^eitschr.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat. 
138:  I,  1932. 

72.  French,  J.  D.,  F.  K.  von  Amerongen  and  H.  W.  Ma- 
GOUN.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  68:  577,  1952. 

73.  French,  J.  D.,  M.  Verzeano  and  H.  W.  M.\goun. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  69:  505,  1953. 

74.  French,  J.  D.,  M.  Verzea.no  and  H.  W.  Magoun. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  69:  519,  1953. 

75.  Gardner,  E.  Anat.  Rec.  83:  401,  1942. 

76.  Gardner,  E.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  80:  11,  1944. 

77.  Gardner,  E.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  152:  436,  1948. 

78.  Gardner,  E.  Anat.  Rec.  loi :  109,  1948. 

79.  Gardner,  E.  Anat.  Rec.  102:  i,  1948. 

80.  Gardner,  E.  Anat.  Rec.  102:  161,  1948. 

81.  Gardner,  E.  Anat.  Rec.  101  :  353,  1948. 

82.  Gardner,  E.  and  H.  M.  Cuneo.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat. 
53:423,  1945. 

83.  Gardner,  E.  and  B.  Hadd.^d.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   172:  475, 

'953- 

84.  Gardner,  E.  and  F.  Morin.  .im.J.  Physml.  174:  149,  1953. 


4^7 


85.  G.ardner,   E.   and  F.   Morin.  Am.  J.  Physiol,    i! 

'957- 

86.  Gardner,  E.  and  R.  Noer.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  168:  437,  1952. 

87.  G.ASSER,  H.  S.  A.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.   15:  35, 
'934- 

88.  Gasser,  H.  S.  A.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc,  23 :  44,  1 943. 

89.  Gasser,  H.  S.  and  J.  Erlanger.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  88:  581, 

1929- 

90.  Gaze,  R.  M.  and  G.  Gordon.  Qiiart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  39: 

279.  1934- 

91.  G.AZE,  R.  M.  and  G.  Gordon.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  40: 

187.  1955- 

92.  Gerard,   R.   VV.,   \V.   H.   Marshall  and   L.  J.   Saul. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  36:  675,  1936. 

93.  Gereetzoff,  M.  a.  Cellule  48:  91,  1939. 

94.  Gernandt,  B.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav. 
12:  56,  1946. 

95.  Gy,t:z,V>.  .\cla  anal.  16:271,  1952. 

96.  Glees,  P.  Verhandl.  Anat.  Gesellsch.  April  1952,  p.  48. 

97.  Glees,  P.,  R.  B.  Livingston  and  J.  Soler.  Archiv  Psychiat. 
187:  190,  1951. 

98.  Glees,   P.   and  J.   Soler.  ^Ischr.  ^ellforsch.  u.  mihoskop. 
Anat.  36:  381,  195!. 

99.  Goldscheider,    A.    Physiologic   des   Muskelsinnes.    Leipzig: 
Barth,  1898. 

100.  Goldstein,  K.  Neurol,  centralbl.  29:  8g8,  1910. 

1 01.  Granit,  R.   Receptors  and  Semory  Perception.  New  Haven 
Yale  Univ.  Press,  1955. 

102.  Granit,  R.  and  H.  D.  Henatsch.  J.  Neurophysiol.  19:  356, 
1956- 

103.  Granit,    R.,    C.   Job   and    B.    R.    Kaada.    Acta  physiol. 
scandinav.  27:  161,  1952. 

104.  Granit,  R.  and  B.  R.  Kaad.\.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  27: 

1^9.  195^- 

105.  Gray,  J.  A.  B.  and  J.  L.  Malcol.m.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London, 
ser.  B  137:96,  1950. 

106.  Grav,  J.  A.  B.  AND  p.  B.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  114: 

454.  1951- 

107.  Gray,  J.  .\.  B.  and  P.  B.  C.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  113: 

475.  '95"  ■ 

108.  Gray, J.  .\.  B.  and  M,  Sato.  J.  Physiol.  122:610,  1953. 

109.  Grant,  F.  C,  R.  A.  Groff  and  F.  H.  Lewy.  Arch.  Neurol. 
&  Psychiat.  43:  498,   1940. 

1 10.  Grant,  F.  C.   and  L.   M.   Weinberger.  Arch.  Surg.  42: 
681,  1 94 1. 

111.  GuiDETTi,  B.  J.  Neurosurg.  7:  499,  1950. 

112.  H.AGBARTH,  K.-E.  AND  D.  L  B.  Kerr.  J.  Neuropliysiol.  17; 

295.  1954- 

1 13.  Hagen,  E.,  H.  Knoche,  D.  C.  Sinclair  and  G.  Weddell. 
Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  141  :  279,  1953. 

114.  Haggqvist,    G.    ^Ischr.     mikroskop.-anat.     Forsch.     39:     i, 
■936. 

115.  Hamby,  \V.  B.,  B.  M.  Shinners  and  J.  A.  Marsh.  Arch. 
Surg.  57:   171,   1948. 

116.  Harwood,  T.  H.  and  R.  H.  Cre.ss.  J.  Neurophysiol.   17: 

157.  '954- 

117.  Hatschek,   R.   Arh.   Inst.   Anat.   u.   Physiol.,    ll'ien.   9:  279, 
1902. 

1 18.  He.ad,  H.  Studies  in  Neurology.  London:  O.vford,  1920. 

119.  Heinbecker,    p.,    G.    H.    Bishop   and  J.    L.   O'Leary. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  29:  771,  1933. 

120.  Hensel,  H.,  and  Y.   Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav. 
22:  96,  1951. 


42t 


12^. 

123. 
124. 
125- 

126. 

127. 
128. 

129. 
130. 

13'- 

132. 

'■Si- 
'34- 

'35- 

136. 
137- 

138. 

139- 
140. 

141. 
142. 

143- 
144. 

145- 
146. 
147. 

148. 
'49- 

150. 

'5'- 

152- 

153- 

154- 
■55- 

.56. 

IS7- 
158. 

'59 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


Hensel,  H.,   and  Y.   Zotterman.   Ada  physiol.  icandinav. 

22:  106,  1 95 1. 

Hensel,  H.,  and  Y.  Zotterman.  AiIu phyuol.  scanditiav.  23: 

29'.  '95'- 

Hensel,  H.,  and  Y.  Zotterman.  J.  \euiophyswt.  14:  377, 

1951- 

Hensel,    H.    and    Y.    Zotterman.    J.    Physiol.    115:    16, 

1951- 

Hern.4ndez-Pe6n,   R.   and   K.-E.   H.^g earth.  J.   \euni- 

physiol.  18:44,  1955. 

Hern.^ndez-Peon,   R.   and   H.  Scherrer.   Fed.  Pmc.    14: 

7'.  '955- 

Hogg,  B.  M.  J.  Phy.tiol.  84:  250,  1935. 

Holmqvist,   B.,   a.   Lundberg   and  O.   Oscarsson.   Acta 

physiol.  scandinav.  38 :  76,  1 956. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  J.  Physiol.  115:  456,  1951. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38:  117,  1954. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  and  S.  \V.  Kuffler.  J.  Physiol.   113:  283, 

■95'- 

Hunt,  C.  C.  and  S.  VV.  Kuffler.  J.  Physiol.   113:  298, 

'95'- 

Hvndman,  O.  R.  Arch.  Surg.  37:  74,  1938. 
Hvndman,  O.  R.  and  J.  VVoLKiN.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychial. 
50:  129,  1943. 

Jasper,  H.  H.  and  C.  Ajmone-Marsan.  A  Slereolaxic  Alias 
of  the  Diencephalon  0/  the  Cat.  Ottawa:  Nat.  Res.  Council 
Canada,  1955. 

Jimenez-C.^stellangs,  J.  J.  Comp.  .\eurol.  91  :  307,  1949. 
King,  E.  E.,  R.  Naquet  .\nd  H.  W.  M.\goun.  J.  Phar- 
macol. &  Exper.   Therap.  119:  48,  1957. 
Knighton,  R.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  92:  183,  1950. 
Kohnstamm,  O.  .Neurol.  Centralbl.  19:  242,  1900. 
Kohnstamm,  O.  J.  Psychol,  u.  .Neurol.  17:  33,  1910. 
Kroll,  F.  W.  ^tschr.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychial.  128:  751,  1930. 
Kruger,  L.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  186:  475,  1956. 
Kruger,  L.  and  p.  Porter.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  In  press. 
Kuffler,  .S.  W.,  C.  C.  Hunt  and  J.  P.  Quilllam.  J. 
Neurophysiol.   14:  29,   1 951. 
Kuhn,  R.  .•X.  TV.  Am.  .N-uml.  .1.   1949,  p.  227. 
KuRU,  M.  Gann  32:  i,  1938. 

KuRU,  M.  Sensory  Paths  in  the  Spinal  Cord  and  Brain  Stem  of 
.Man.  Tokyo  and  Osaka:  Sogensya,  1949. 
Landau,  VV.  M.  Science  123:  895,  1956. 
Land.au,    W.    M.    and   G.    H.    Bishop.    Arch.    .Neurol.   & 
Psychial.  69:490,  1953. 

Laporte,  Y.  and  a.  Lundberg.  .icla  physiol.  scandinav.  36: 
204,  1956. 

Laporte,  Y.,  A.  Lundberg  and  O.  Oscarsson.  Ada 
physiol.  scandinav.  36 :  1 75,  1 956. 

Laporte,   Y.,   A.   Lundberg   and   O.    Oscarsson.    Acta 
physiol.  scandinav.  36 :  1 88,  1 956. 
Leksell,    L.    .4cta  physiol.    .•scandinav.    Suppl.    31,    10:    i, 

1945- 

Lele,  P.  P.  and  G.  Weddell.  Brain  79 :  119,  1 956. 

Lele,  P.  P.,  G.  Weddell  and  C.  M.  Williams.  J.  Physiol. 

126:  206,  1954. 

Lenhossek,  M.   Der  Feinere  Ban  des  .Nrrvensystems  in  Lichte 

Neuster  Forschungen.  Berlin:  Fischer,   1895. 

Li,  C.-L.,  C.  Cullen  and  H.  H.   J.asper.  J.  .\europhysiol. 

19:  III,  1956. 

Li,  C.-L,  C.  Cullen  and  H.  H.  J.^sper.  J.  .\europhysiol. 

'9-  13".  1956- 

Li,  C.-L  and  H.  H.  Jasper.  J.  Physiol.  iz\  :  1 17,  1953. 


60.  Li.ovD,  D.   P.  C.  AND  A.  K.  McIntvre.  J    .\europhysiol. 

13:  39.  1950- 

61.  LoEWENSTEiN,  VV.  R.J.  Physiol.  133:588,  1956. 

62.  LoRENTE  DE  N6,  R.   In:  Physiology  0/  the  .Nervous  System, 
edited  by  J.  F.  Fulton.  New  York:  Oxford,  1949. 

63.  Lundberg,  A.  and  O.  Oscarsson.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav. 

38:  353.  '956- 

64.  Macoun,  H.  W.  and  \V.  .\.  McKinley.  .-Im.  J.  Physiol. 

■37:  409.  '942- 

65.  Malis,  L.  I.,  K.  H.  Pribram  .^nd  L.  Kruger.  J.  .Neuro- 
physiol.  16:  161,  1953. 

66.  Marshall,  VV.  H.  J.  .Xeurophysiol .  4:  25,  1941. 

67.  Marshall,   W.   H.,    C.    N.    VVoolsev   .\nd   P.    Bard.   J. 
.Neurophyswl .  4 :  1 ,  1 94 1 . 

68.  Maruhashi,  J.,  K.  Mizuguchi  .\nd  I.  T.asaki.  J.  Physiol. 
"7:  129,  1952. 

69.  Matthews,  B.  H.  C.  J.  Physiol.  78:  i,  1933. 

70.  M.'\TZKE.  H.  .\.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  94:  439,  1951. 

71.  McIntyre,  a.  K.  Proc.  Univ.  Otago  31:5,  1953. 

72.  McKinley,  VV.  A.  and  H.  VV.  Magoun.  .im.  J.  Physiol. 
'37:  217,  1942. 

73.  Mehler,  VV.  R.  .inat.  Rec.  127:  332,  1957. 

74.  Monnier,  M.  Ada  neurochir.  Suppl.  3:  291,  1955. 

75.  Monnier,  M.  Rev.  neurol.  93:  267,  1956. 

76.  MoRiN,  F.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.  183:  245,  1955. 

77.  MoRiN,  F.   AND  J.  V.   Cat.alano.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.    103: 

'7.  '955- 

78.  MoRiN,  F.,  H.  G.  Schwartz  and  J.  L.  O'Leary.  .icla 
psychial.  el  neurol.  scandinav.  26:  371,  1951. 

79.  MoRuzzi,   G.   and   H.   VV.   M.agoun.   Eleclroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  .Neurophyswl.  i  :  455,  1 949. 

80.  MoTT,  F.  VV.  Brain  18:  i,  1895. 

81.  MouNTCASTLE,  V.  B.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  20:  408,  1957. 

82.  MoUNTCASTLE,  V.  B.,  M.  R.  COVIAN  AND  C.  R.  Harrison. 
.A.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  339,  1950. 

83.  MoUNTCASTLE,    V.    B.,    P.    W.    DaVIES   .AND    A.    L.    BeRMAN. 

J.  .\europhysiol.  20:  374,  1957. 

84.  MoUNTCASTLE,  V.  B.  .»iND  E.  Hennem.^n.  J.  .\'europhysiol. 

12:  85,  1949. 

85.  MoUNTCASTLE,  V.  B.  AND  E.  Hennem.an.  J.  Comp.  .\eiirol. 

97:  409.  ■952- 

86.  Nathan,  P.  VV.  and  M.  C.  Smith.  J.  .\rurol.  .\eurosurg.  & 
Psychial.  18:  181,  1955. 

87.  Ness,  .\.  R.J.  Physiol.  126:  475,  1954. 

88.  Olivecrona,  H.  .irch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychial.  47:  544,  1942. 

89.  Olszewski,   J.    The    Thalamus   of  Macaco    Mulalla.    New 
York:  S.  Karger,   1952. 

90.  Oscarsson,  O.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  38:  144,  1956. 

91.  Papez,  J.  VV.  .inat.  Rec.  109:  405,  1951. 

92.  Papez,  J.   VV.   and  W.   Rundles.  J.  .Nerv.  &  Menl.   Dis. 

85:505.  '937- 

93.  P.\TTON,  H.  D.  AND  V.  E.  Am.\ssian.  .im.  J.  Physiol.  183: 

650.  1955- 

94.  Pearson,  A.  .\.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychial.  68:  515,  1952. 

95.  Pease,  D.  C.  and  T.  A.  Quilliam.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem. 
Cylol.  3:  331,   1957. 

96.  Petren,  K.  Arch.  Psychial.  47:  495,  1910. 

97.  Pfaffmann,  C.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  17:  243,  1941. 

98.  Powell,  T.   P.   S.   and  V.   B.   Mountc.\stle.   Fed.  Proc. 
17:   126,  1958. 

199.  Pribram,   H.   B.   and  J.   Barry.  J.  .Neurophysiol.    19:  99, 

1956- 

200.  Probst,  M.  .irch.  Psychial.  33:  i,   1900. 


TOUCH    AND    KINESTHESIS 


4^9 


201.  Ram6n    ^'    Cajal,    S.    Hislologie    du    Systeme    .\'erveux    de 
r Homme  ft  des  Vertebres.  Paris:  Maloine,  1909. 

202.  Raney,   R.,   a.   a.   Raney  and   C.   R.   Hunter.   Am.  J. 
Surg.  80:  II,  1950. 

203.  Ranson,  S.  VV.  and  VV.  R.  Ingram.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  56: 

^57.  '93^- 

204.  Rasmussen,  a.  T.   .■^nd  W.  T.   Peyton.  .Surgery   10:  699, 

1941- 

205.  Rexed,  B.  and  a.  Brodal.  J.  Neurophysiot .  14:  399,  1951. 

206.  Rexed,   B.   .and   G.    Strom.    Acta  physiol.    scandinav.    25: 
219,  1952. 

207.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle.  J.  Comp.  .\eurot.  97 : 

441.  195^- 

208.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountcastle.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hasp.  94:  238,  1954. 

209.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  Biological  and  Biochemical 
Bases  oj  Behavior.  Madison:  Univ.  Wisconsin  Press.  In  press. 

210.  Ruch,  T.  C.  and  J.  F.  Fulton.  A.  Res.  .\erv.  &  .Merit.  Dis., 
Proc.  15:288,  1933. 

211.  Ruch,  T.  C,  J.  F.  Fulton  and  W.  J.  German,  .-irch. 
Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  39:  919,  1938. 

212.  Ruch,  T.  C.,  J.  F.  Fulton  and  S.  Kasdon.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

"9:  394.  ■937- 

213.  Ruch,  T.  C,  S.  Kasdon  and  J.  F.  Fulton,  .im.  J.  Phynol. 

'^9    453.  1940- 

214.  Russell,  G.  V.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  loi  :  237,  1954. 

215.  S.AMUEL,  E.  P.  .4nal.  Rec.  1 13:  53,  1952. 

216.  Sasaoka,  S.  Jap.  J.  Med.  Sc.  I.  Anal.  7:  315,  1939. 

217.  Scherrer,  H.   and  R.  Hern.andez-Peon.  Fed.  Proc.    14: 

132,  1955- 

218.  .Schwartz,   H.   G.   and   J.   L.   O'Leary.  .Surgery  g:    183, 

1941- 

219.  Schwartz,  H.  G.   and   J.   L.  O'Leary.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  47:  293,  1942. 

220.  Sinclair,  D.  C.  Brain  78:  584,  1955. 

221.  Sinclair,    D.    C.    and    J.    R.    Hinsh.aw.    Brain    73:    224, 

■950- 

222.  Sinclair,    D.    C.    and  J.    R.    Hinshaw.    Brain    73:    480, 

■95°- 

223.  Sinclair,  D.  C..  G.  Weddell  and  E.  Zander.  J.  .inal. 
86:  402,  1952. 

224.  SjOQViST,  O.  .Acta  psychiat.  el  neurol.  .Svippl.   17,  1938. 

225.  Skoglund,   S.   .icta  physiol.  scandinav.   Suppl.    124,   36:    1, 
'956. 

226.  Sperry,  R.  W.  J.  Neurophysiol.  10:  275,   1947. 

227.  Sperry,  R.  W.,  N.  Minor  and  R.  E.  Myers.  J.  Comp.  & 
Physiol.  Psychol.  48:  50,  1955. 

228.  Stamm,  J.   S.   and   R.   VV.   Sperry.  J.   Comp.  &  Physiol. 
Psychol.  50:  138,   1957. 

229.  Starzl,  T.   E.,   C.  W.  Taylor   and  H.   \V.   Magoun. 
J.  Neurophysiol.  14:  461,  1951. 

230.  Starzl,   T.    E.,   C.   W.   Taylor   and   H.    W.    Magoun. 
J.  Neurophysiol.  14:  479,  1951. 

231.  Stilwell,  D.  J.,  Jr.  Anat.  Rec.  127:  635,  1957. 

232.  Stilwell,  D.  L.,  Jr.  Am.  J.  Anat.  100:  289,  1957. 

233.  Stilwell,  D.  L.,  Jr.  Am.  J.  Anat.  loi :  59,  1957. 

234.  Stopford,  J.  S.  B.  J.  Anat.  59:  120,  1925. 

235.  Stratford,  J.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  100:  i,  1954. 

236.  Sweet,  VV.  H.„  J.  C.  White,  B.  Selverstone  and  R. 
Nilges.  Tr.  Am.  Neurol.  A.  1950,  p.  165. 

237.  Szentagothai,  J.  AND  T.  Kiss.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat. 

6^:  734.  1949- 

238.  ToRViK,  A.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  106:  51,  1956. 


239.  ToRviK,  A.  Am.  J.  Anat.  100:  i,  1957. 

240.  TscHERMAK,  A.  Ncurol.  Centralbl.  17:  159,  1898. 

241.  VAN  Gehuchten,  a.  Nevraxe  3:  235,  1901. 

242.  VoGT,  C.  J.  Psychol,  u.  Neurol.  12:  285,  1909. 

243.  VON    EcONOMO,    C.    Jahrh.    Psychiat.    u.    Neurol.    32:    107, 
1911. 

244.  VON  Fre\-,  M.  Ber.  sachs.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  46:  185,  1894. 

245.  VON  Fre'i-,  M.  Ber.  sdchs.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  46:  283,  1894. 

246.  VON  Frey,  M.  Ber.  sUchs.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  47:  166,  1895. 

247.  VON  Fre\-,  M.  Abh.  sdchs.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  40:  175,  1896. 

248.  Walberg,  F.  Brain  80:  273,  1957. 

249.  W.alker,  a.  E.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  60:  161,  1934. 

250.  Walker,  A.  E.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  64:  i,  1936. 

251.  VV'alker,  .\.  E.  Koninkl.  Nad.  .ikad.   Wetenschap.,  Proc.  40: 

'98.  1937- 

252.  Walker,  .\.  E.  Confinia  .\eurol.  i  :  99,  1938. 

253.  Walker,   A.    E.    The  Primate    Thalamus.    Chicago:    Univ. 
Chicago  Press,  1938. 

254.  Walker,  .\.  E.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  71  :  59,  1939. 

255.  Walker,  .A.  E.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  43:  284,   1940. 

256.  Walker,  A.  E.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  48:  865,  1942. 

257.  Walker,  A.  E.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  48:  884,  1942. 

258.  Walker,  A.  E.  and  T.  A.  VVe.aver,  Jr.  J.  Comp.  .\eiirol. 
76:  145,  1942. 

259.  Wallenberg,  A.  Anat.  Anz-  12:  95,  1896. 

260.  Wallenberg,  .A.  Anat.  Anz.  18:  81,  1900. 

261.  Wallenberg,  A.  .innt.  Anr.  26:  145,  1905. 

262.  Walshe,  F.  M.  R.  Brain  63:  48,  1942. 

263.  Weaver,  T.  A.,  Jr.,  and  A.  E.  Walker.  Arch.  .\'eiirol.  & 
Psychiat.  46:  877,  1941 . 

264.  Weddell,  G.  J.  Anat.  75:  346,  1941. 
263.  Weddell,  G.  J.  Anat.  73:  441,  1941. 

266.  Weddell,  G.  and  J.  A.  Harpman.  J.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat. 
3:  319,  1940. 

267.  Weddell,    G.,    W.    Pallie    and    E.    Palmer.    Quail.   J. 
Micro.ic.  Sc.  93:  483,  1954. 

268.  Weddell,  G.,  E.  Palmer  and  W.  Pallie.  Biol.  Rev.  30: 

■59.  '955- 

269.  Weddell,  G.,  D.  A.  T.aylor  and  C.  M.  Williams.  J. 
Anat.  89:  317,   1955. 

270.  Weinberger,  L.  M.   and  F.  C.  Grant.  Arch.  .Xeurol.  & 
Psychiat.  48:  333,  1942. 

271.  White,  J.  C.  Arch.  Surg.  43:  113,  1941. 

272.  White,  J.  C,  E.  P.  Richardson,  Jr.,  and  W.  H.  Sweet. 
Ann.  Surg.  144:  407,  1936. 

273.  White,  J.  C.  and  VV.  H.  Swtet.  Pain.  Its  .Mechanisms  and 
Neurosurgical  Control.  .Springfield:  Thomas,   1935. 

274.  White,  J.  C,  W.  H.  Sweet,  R.  Hawkins  and  R.   G. 
Nilges.  Brain  73:  346,  1950. 

273.  Woollard,  H.  H.,  G.  Weddell  and  J.  A.   Harpman. 
J.  Anat.  74:  413,  1940. 

276.  Woolsey,  C.  N.,  H.-T.  Chang  and  P.  Bard.  Fed.  Proc. 
6:  230,  1947. 

277.  Woolsey,  C.  N.  and  D.  FAiRM.-iN.  Surgery  19:  684,   1946. 

278.  Woolsey,  C.  N.  .\nd  G.  H.  Wang.  Fed.  Proc.  4:  79,  1945. 

279.  Wrete,  M.  Acta  Anat.  7:  173,  1949. 

280.  Yamamoto,   S.,   S.   Sugihara,   and   M.   Kuru.  Jap.  J. 
Physiol.  6:68,  1956. 

281.  Zotterman,  Y.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  73:  103,  1936. 

282.  Zotterman,  Y.  J.  Physiol.  93:  i,  1939. 

283.  ZuBEK,  J.  p.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  44:  339,  1931. 

284.  ZuBEK,  J.  V .  J .  Neurophysiol .  15:401,  1951. 
283.   Zubek,  J.  P.  Canad.  J.  Psychol.  6:  183,  1932. 

286.   Zubek,  J.  P.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  43:  438,  1932. 


CHAPTER     XVIII 


Thermal  sensations 


YNGVE    ZOTTERMAN     1      Department  of  Physiology,  Veterindrhogskolan,  Stockholm,  Sweden 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 


STRUCTURE    OF    RECEPTIVE    FIELD 


Structure  of  Receptive  Field 

Topography  of  Thermal  Senses:  Cold  and  Warm  Spots 
Depth  of  Thermal  Nerve  Endings 
Identification  of  Thermal  Receptors 
Afferent  Nerve  Paths 
Conditions  for  Thermal  Sensations 
Conduction  of  Heat  in  Skin 
Relation  Between  Temperature  Change  Recorded  in  Skin 

and  Thermal  Sensation 
Paradoxical  Sensations 
Thermal  After -Sensations 
Sensation  of  Hot' 
Electrophysioiogy  of  Thermal  Nerve  Fibers 
Specificity  of  Nerve  Fibers  in  Mammals 
Thermal  Receptors  in  Cold-Blooded  Animals 
Quantitative   Relations   Between    Temperature    Movements 
and  Nerve  Fiber  Discharge 
Methods 

Discharge  at  constant  temperature 
Cold  fibers 
Warm  fibers 
Response  of  thermal  receptors  to  temperature  changes 
Cold  fibers 
Warm  fibers 
Paradoxical  discharges 
Effect  of  temperatures  above  47  °C 
Intracutaneous  gradient 

Response  of  mechanoreceptors  to  thermal  stimulation 
Influence  of  Nonthermal  Agencies 
Theoretical  Considerations 
Central  Threshold 
Excitation  Mechanism  of  Thermal  Receptors 


The  different  sensations  of  cold  and  warmth  are  produced  by 
stimulation  of  separate  specific  nerve  end-organs  in  the  skin. 

Magnus  Blix  1882  (9). 


Topography  of  Thermal  Senses:  Cold  and  ]Varm  Spots 

SINCE  THE  DISCOVERY  by  Blix  (9,  I o)  ol"  cold  and  warm 
spots  from  which  adequate  or  electrical  stimuli  elic- 
ited cold  and  warm  sensations,  respectively,  numerous 
authors  have  described  the  distribution  of  cold  and 
warm  spots  in  the  skin  as  well  as  in  the  mucous  mem- 
branes of  man.  In  general  cold  spots  are  far  more 
numerous  than  warm  spots,  but  the  relation  between 
the  density  of  the  two  kinds  of  temperature  sensitive 
spots  varies  a  good  deal  in  different  areas.  Hensel  (45) 
in  his  review  emphasizes  the  great  errors  inherent  in 
finding  these  thermal  spots  by  using  punctiform 
stimuli  such  as  Blix's  cone  affords.  The  highest  density 
of  thermosensitive  spots  is  found  in  some  areas  of  the 
face.  Particularly  sensitive  to  thermal  stimulation  are 
the  eyelids  and  the  lips.  The  forehead  is  very  cold- 
sensitive  but  only  moderately  sensitive  to  warmth. 
The  hairy  parts  of  the  head,  the  patellar  region  and 
the  tongue  are  very  slightly  sensitive  to  warmth.  The 
conjuctiva  bulbi  and  the  periphery  of  the  cornea 
possess  cold  sensitivity  but  do  not  respond  to  warmth. 
Careful  investigations  on  the  distribution  of  tempera- 
ture spots  have  been  made  for  the  whole  body  by 
Rein  (72)  and  Goldscheider  (32),  for  the  genital  or- 
gans by  Hauer  (39),  Speiser  (81)  and  Beetz  (6),  for 
the  eye  by  Strughold  &  Karge  (84)  and  Strughold  & 
Porz  (85),  and  for  the  mucous  membranes  of  the 
mouth  and  the  nose  by  Rein  (72),  Strughold  (83), 
Schriever  &  Strughold  (jS)  and  Hirsch  &  Schriever 
(59).  In  these  papers  as  well  as  in  Goldscheider's  re- 
view (32)  topographical  charts  of  temperature  spots 
will  be  found.  In  table  i  the  mean  density  of  cold 
and  warm  spots  is  given  for  different  areas  of  the  body 
surface.  The  high  temperature  sensitivity  of  the  tri- 


43' 


43^ 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


TABLE  I  Distrihiitinn  (if  Warm  nrul  Cold  Spots 
in  Human  Skin* 


Forehead  

Nose 

Lips 

Other  parts  of  face 

Chest 

Abdomen 

Back 

Upper   arm 

Forearm 

Back  of  hand 

Palm  of  hand 

Finger  dorsal 

Finger  volar 

Thigh 

Calf 

Back  of  foot 

Sole  of  foot 


Cold  Spotst 

Warm  Spots} 

5-5-8 

8-13 

I 

16-19 

8.5-9 

1-7 

9-10.2 

0-3 

8-12.5 

7-8 

5-6-5 

6-7-5 

0.3-0.4 

7-4 

0-5 

1-5 

0.4 

7-9 

1-7 

2-4 

1.6 

4-5-5-2 

0.4 

4-3-5-7 

5-6 

3-4 

*  Number  per  cm-. 

t  After  Strughold  &  Porz  (85). 

I  After  Rein  (72). 


geminal  area  which  in  man  is  directly  exposed  to  all 
weathers  no  doubt  has  special  importance. 

Concerning  the  thermal  sensitivity  of  animals  our 
knowledge  is  very  scarce  and  scattered.  Until  recently 
cold-blooded  animals  were  not  believed  to  possess  any 
specific  thermoceptive  organs.  Sand  (77)  using  elec- 
trophysiological methods  discovered  that  the  Loren- 
zinian  ampullae  of  Raja  reacted  to  cooling.  The 
Lorenzinian  ampullae  of  the  elasmobranchs  are 
situated  laterally  in  the  region  of  the  head  and  con- 
sist of  a  group  of  small  mucous  cysts  lying  subcu- 
taneously.  They  are  supplied  by  afferent  fibers  from 
the  facial  nerve. 

The  facial  pits  of  the  pit  viper  (Crotalidae),  which 
originally  were  believed  to  function  as  mechano- 
ceptors  specialized  for  the  detection  of  air  vibrations, 
were  clearly  shown  by  Noble  &  Schmidt  (70)  through 
behavioral  experiments  to  detect  the  body  tempera- 
ture of  the  snakes'  prey.  They  proved  that  snakes 
with  the  other  principal  sense  organs  of  the  head 
nonfunctional  can  still  strike  correctly  at  moving  ob- 
jects and  can  discriminate  between  warm  and  cold 
ones  as  long  as  the  pits  are  uncovered.  The  organ 
consists  of  a  small  pit  about  3  mm  in  diameter  covered 
by  a  membrane  15  /z  thick.  This  thin  membrane  is 
the  innervated  sensory  surface.  Leading  off  from 
microelectrodes,  steel  needles  with  tip  diameter  of 
al)out  3  to  7  ;u  inserted  into  the  membrane,  Bullock 


&  Cowles  (12),  Bullock  &  Diecke  (13)  and  Bullock  & 
Faulstick  (14)  pro\ed  that  the  afferent  nerve  endings 
serv-e  as  infrared  receptors.  They  are,  so  far  as  we 
know  at  pre.sent,  the  most  densely  distributed  warm 
receptors  and  the  most  effective  organ  for  infrared 
detection  within  the  animal  kingdom.  In  mammals 
cold  sensiti\ity  seems  to  be  located  particularily  on 
the  bare  parts  of  the  nose  and  on  the  tip  of  the 
tongue.  More  details  are  not  available  as  yet. 

Depth  of  Thermal  Nerve  Endings 

The  fact  that  the  reaction  time  for  warmth  is  con- 
sistently longer  than  that  for  cold  suggested  that  the 
warm  receptors  should  be  located  deeper  in  the  skin 
than  the  cold  receptors  (87,  91).  This  assumption  had 
many  proponents  (i,  26,  27,  72).  Bazett  et  al.  (5) 
calculated  the  depth  of  the  thermal  receptors  in  the 
prepuce.  The  skin  was  stretched  out  into  a  flat  sheet 
by  means  of  small  ijari^less  fish-hooks.  Sensitive  spots 
belonging  to  one  layer  of  skin  could  thus  be  stimu- 
lated from  either  side  of  the  double  fold.  The  rate  of 
transmission  of  the  temperature  wave  through  the 
fold  was  determined  by  thermoelectrical  recording; 
the  value  obtained  of  about  i  mm  per  sec.  is  in  agree- 
ment with  more  recent  measurements  of  Hensel  & 
Zotterman  (55).  From  this  figure  and  the  reaction 
time  of  the  subject  so  stimulated  it  was  possible  to 
estimate  the  depth  of  the  receptors. 

The  average  depth  of  the  warm  receptors  was  thus 
found  to  be  0.3  mm.  For  the  cold  receptors  the  aver- 
age depth  was  computed  to  somewhat  less  than  0.17 
mm.  The  depths  of  the  receptors  for  cold  and  warmth 
computed  in  this  way  were  in  good  agreement  with 
the  average  depth  of  the  Krause  and  the  Ruftini  type 
of  end  organ  respectively  as  determined  histologically. 

This  and  other  previous  methods  based  upon  the 
subjective  reaction  time  to  thermal  stimuli  must  be 
subject  to  rather  large  errors  because  a  great  number 
of  uncontrollal)le  reactions  take  place  between  the 
application  of  the  stimulus  and  the  conscious  action 
of  the  subject,  the  time  of  which  is  many  times  longer 
than  that  of  the  actual  peripheral  events  occurring  in 
the  thermal  receptors  themselves. 

By  using  the  method  of  recording  the  spike  poten- 
tials in  the  specific  cold  fibers  Hensel  et  al.  CoO 
developed  a  method  of  physiological  depth  determina- 
tion which  eliminates  the  errors  of  the  methods  previ- 
ously used.  The  method  has  been  used  for  determining 
the  depth  of  the  cold  receptors  on  the  tip  of  the  tongue 
of  the  cat  and  the  dog  but  can  of  course  in  principle 
be  applied  even  to  human  suijjects. 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


433 


The  principle  of  the  method  consists  of  determining 
by  means  of  small  rectangular  temperature  steps  the 
threshold  temperature  change,  d„  for  the  cold  recep- 
tors. A  large  well-defined  cold  pulse  was  applied  then 
to  the  surface  by  means  of  a  special  thermode  de- 
scribed in  figure  14;  then  the  lapse  of  time,  /,  from 
the  beginning  of  the  pulse  until  the  appearance  of  the 
first  cold  fiber  spike  was  measured.  The  tiine,  t,  is 
composed  of  the  "thermal  latency,'  te,  the  time  which 
the  cold  needs  to  penetrate  into  the  receptor  layer, 
and  two  constants:  the  nerve  conduction  time,  <„,  of 
the  cold  fibers  and  the  physiological  latency,  /r,  of  the 
cold  receptor.  For  the  thermal  latency,  te,  from  which 
the  depth  of  the  receptor  can  be  calculated,  we 
obtain 


te 


tr- 


When  the  thermal  diffusion  coefficient,  a,  of  the  living 
skin  is  known,  it  is  possible  to  calculate  to  what  depth 
the  threshold  temperature  change,  d„  has  proceeded 
within  the  time,  te-  This  depth  is  the  depth  of  the 
receptor.  By  means  of  a  double  beam  cathode  ray 
oscillograph  for  simultaneous  recording  of  the  electric 
response  from  the  cold  fibers  and  the  temperature  of 
the  surface  of  the  tongue,  the  beginning  of  the  tem- 
perature course  was  easily  determined  with  an  ac- 
curacy of  ±0.002  sec. 

Figure  i  shows  a  record  of  the  discharge  of  cold 
spikes  from  a  strand  of  the  lingual  nerve  of  the  cat  in 
response  to  a  sudden  cooling  from  38  to  I5°C  and 
rewarming.  Simultaneously  the  temperature  of  the 
silver  bottom  of  a  thermode  on  the  tongue  was  re- 
corded by  the  second  beam.  After  an  interval,  /  = 
0.023  sec.,  from  the  beginning  of  the  cooling,  the  first 
action  potential  from  the  cold  fibers  appeared.  On 
rewarming,  the  last  cold  fiber  spike  disappeared  after 


an  interval  of  0.027  ^^c.  from  the  beginning  of  the 
rewarming.  The  determinations  of  /  were  repeated 
several  times  for  each  preparation.  As  was  shown  by 
Zotterman  (96)  the  shorter  the  interval,  /,  the  larger 
the  temperature  steps.  For  the  preparation  of  figure 
I,  for  example,  values  of  /  between  0.015  ^^^-  (for 
steps  from  38  to  5°C)  and  0.07  sec.  (for  steps  from  38 
to  34°C)  were  obtained.  For  the  sum  of  the  two  con- 
stants /„  and  tr  an  interval  of  about  0.006  sec.  was 
computed.  The  latency  of  the  cold  receptors,  about 
0.003  to  0.005  sec,  is  obtained  by  comparing  the 
intervals,  /,  at  large  and  small  temperature  steps. 
Using  this  value  of  /r  we  obtained  exactly  the  same 
depth  at  all  temperature  steps;  at  larger  values  of  tr 
the  values  of  the  depth  were  too  small  compared  to 
the  values  computed  when  using  medium  or  small 
temperature  steps.  From  about  70  separate  measure- 
ments on  six  cats  the  following  values  were  obtained : 


Relatise 

threshold      -  - 
Average  depth 

of  receptors 
Dispersion 


0-5 


o.  It  mm 


0.18  mm 


0.20  mm 


±0.015  n^"^       ±0.015  "^"^       ±0.018  mm 


The  physiological  depth  determinations  of  the  cold 
receptors  are  in  good  accordance  with  the  histological 
observations  made  on  serial  slides  from  the  same  area 
of  the  cat  tongue.  The  epithelium  of  the  papillae  has 
a  height  of  0.05  to  0.08  mm.  The  musculature  of  the 
tongue  starts  with  a  rather  sharply  defined  border 
line  at  a  depth  of  about  0.3  mm.  Closely  above  the 
musculature  of  the  tongue  there  is  a  well-developed 
net  of  blood  vessels.  Thus,  according  to  these  deter- 
minations, the  cold  receptors  are  situated  subepi- 
thelially  partly  in  the  papillae  and  particularly  at  their 
base  or  just  beneath  them. 


.^ 

"l 

Cooling 

\              '         1 

-Nil  liiiiikifcu^  'iiiiiite  i 

"Rewarming 

1      ' 

iO°C 

50 

10 

20 

't     ,     1 

II 

■          1          1          1 

0 

,T 

r        1        I 

FIG.  I .  Simultaneous  records  of  cold  potentials  in  a  fine  strand  of  cat  lingual  nerve  and  of  tempera- 
ture of  silver  bottom  of  thermode  on  tongue  during  sudden  cooling  from  about  40°  to  I5°C  and 
rewarming.  Lejt  temperature  scale  for  cooling,  right  scale  for  rewarming.  Time,  50  cps.  [From  Hensel 


434 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Identification  of  Thermal  Receptors 

Since  the  recording  of  spike  activity  of  single 
thermal  nerve  fibers  by  Zotterman  and  co-workers 
(22,  52,  96),  there  can  be  no  doubt  about  the  speci- 
ficity of  cold  and  warm  nerve  endings  in  the  mammals 
as  these  thermal  receptors  discharging  into  func- 
tionally isolated  nerve  fibers  responded  to  cooling  and 
warming,  respectively,  but  not  to  mechanical  stimu- 
lation. The  abundance  of  information  about  the  func- 
tion of  the  thermal  receptors  obtained  in  recent  years 
from  electrophysiological  investigations  has,  howev-er, 
not  been  followed  by  any  corresponding  widening  of 
our  knowledge  of  the  morphological  structure  of  the 
receptors.  The  old  attempts  to  identify  the  receptors 
histologically  h\  e.xcision  of  human  skin  beneath  the 
cold  and  warm  spots,  respectively,  failed  almost  en- 
tirely [see  von  Skramlik  (92)].  The  statement  in  most 
textbooks  that  the  Krause  end  bulbs  are  believed  to  be 
the  receptor  for  cold  and  the  Ruffini  end  organ  that 
for  warmth  were  based  on  histological  studies  by  von 
Frey  (91)  and  Strughold  &  Karbe  (84)  on  sensory 
end  organs  within  the  cold  sensitive  periphery  of  the 
cornea.  After  mapping  the  cold  spots  on  the  conjunc- 
tiva bulbi,  Strughold  &  Karbe  dropped  methylene 
Ijlue  into  the  eye  and  found  a  very  good  topographical 
correlation  between  the  cold  spots  and  the  blue 
stained  end  bulbs  observed  in  the  corneal  microscope. 
Similarly  Bazett  et  al.  (5)  in  their  attempt  to  identify 
the  end  organs  for  temperature  and  touch  in  the  pre- 
puce injected  methylene  blue  intra-arterially.  They 
described  seven  different  types  of  end  organs,  among 
which  end  bulbs  of  the  Krause  type  were  distributed 
in  good  agreement  with  the  cold  spots.  Their  average 
number  was  about  15  per  cm-  compared  with  6  to  12 
for  the  cold  spots,  but  some  of  the  end  bulbs  were  so 
close  together  that  their  number  obviously  must 
exceed  that  found  by  mapping  the  cold  spots.  Further, 
there  are  reasons  to  believe  that  some  nerve  fibers 
branch  and  supply  more  than  one  end  organ.  Spots 
sensitive  to  warmth  in  the  prepuce  are  few  in  number 
(one  or  rather  less  per  cm-).  The  distribution  of  the 
Ruffini  end  organs  agreed  fairly  well  with  that  of  the 
warm  spots. 

Whether  the  Krause  end  bulbs  are  the  receptors 
for  cold  in  other  parts  of  the  skin  is  still  uncertain 
since  conventional  histological  methods  have  failed  to 
reveal  any  end  bulbs  of  the  Krause  type  in  the  skin 
underlying  the  cold  spots.  Recently  Lele  et  al.  (65) 
maintained  that,  in  limited  areas  of  the  skin  (as  opposed 
to  mucous  membranes)  in  which  encapsulated  nerve 
endings  are  abundant  (the  palm  of  the  hand,  sole  of 
the  foot  and  parts  of  the  dorsum  of  the  digits),  the 


diversity  in  size  and  configuration  of  their  cellular 
and  neural  elements  is  such  that  any  classification  of 
encapsulated  endings  in  the  skin  becomes  purely 
arbitrary.  On  the  other  hand  they  draw  attention  to 
the  fact  that  in  both  glabrous  and  hairy  skin  en- 
sheathed  nerve  fillers  arising  from  the  cutaneous  nerve 
ple.xus  give  rise  at  all  levels  in  the  skin  (from  the 
stratum  granulosum  of  the  epidermis  to  the  junction 
of  the  dermis  and  the  subcutaneous  tissues)  to  a  wide- 
spread series  of  fine  naked  axoplasmic  filaments 
which  interweave  but  do  not  fuse  with  one  another. 
These  unencapsulated  nerve  endings  cannot  be  dis- 
tinguished from  one  another  on  morphological 
grounds;  they  can  be  distinguished  only  by  the  fact 
that  they  are  situated  in  a  different  stratum  of  the 
skin  and  thus  lie  among  different  tissue  elements. 
Thus  the  morphologically  nonspecific  nerve  endings 
found  beneath  the  epithelium  should  be  reduced  in 
temperature  and  therefore  be  stimulated  by  cooling 
of  the  skin.  The  deeper  endings  situated  close  to  the 
lilood  vessels  are  generally  heated  up  by  the  blood  so 
that  a  positive  temperature  gradient  between  the 
ending  and  the  axons  should  be  the  usual  mode  of 
stimulation.  According  to  Lele  et  al.  the  temperature 
modes  are  related  not  to  the  stimulation  of  morpho- 
logically specific  endings,  but  to  the  manner  in  which 
nonspecific  nerve  endings  of  fibers  in  the  skin  are 
stimulated.  These  unencapsulated  endings  should 
thus,  according  to  these  authors,  be  looked  upon  "as 
universal  receptors  which  give  rise  to  bursts  of  action 
potentials,  the  pattern  of  which  is  related  to  the  way 
in  which  the  stimulus  affects  the  skin."  Consequently 
they  also  maintain  that  Johannes  Miiller's  'law  of 
specific  energies'  and  the  thesis  that  there  are  specific 
nerve  terminals  which  subserve  specific  sensory 
modalities  is  unsupported. 

The  absence  of  encapsulated  nerve  endings  does, 
however,  not  exclude  the  possibility  of  functional 
specificity.  The  tongue  of  the  frog  does  not  contain 
any  such  endings  although  it  contains  afferent  fibers 
responding  specifically  to  touch,  salt  and  water  (98). 
In  recent  years  it  has  often  been  suggested  that  the 
capsule  of  an  end  organ,  for  example  in  a  Pacinian 
corpuscle,  protects  the  nerve  ending  from  being 
damaged  when  the  organ  is  subjected  to  strong  and 
lasting  mechanical  stimulation  as  in  the  beak  of  a 
wood  pecker.  The  capsule  should  thus  have  nothing 
specifically  to  do  with  the  energy  transformation. 
This  would  imply  that  the  specific  process  of  transfor- 
mation should  be  inherent  in  the  morphologically  non- 
specific naked  nerve  endings  or  in  the  structures 
where  these  are  situated.  The  sensation  experienced 


THERMAL   SENSATIONS 


435 


upon  stimulation  of  the  nerve  ending  may  be  more 
closely  correlated  with  the  most  usual  mode  of  stimu- 
lation— which,  for  the  superficially  situated  'cold' 
endings,  is  cooling  of  the  skin.  The  discharges  from 
these  endings  are  then  transmitted  to  specific  cells  of 
the  cortex  the  activity  of  which  will  be  labelled  cold. 
So  far  it  is  easy  to  follow  the  idea  of  Lele  et  al.  (65). 
But  when  these  authors  maintain  that  the  thermal 
nerve  endings  respond  even  to  mechanical  and  noxi- 
ous stimuli  they  diverge  from  the  experimental  evi- 
dence, in  that  the  activity  of  single  temperature  fibers 
of  the  cat  and  the  dog  cannot  be  influenced  by  me- 
chanical stimulation  of  their  receptive  field  at  least 
within  reasonable  limits  of  stimulus  strength.  The 
nonspecific  response  of  certain  mechanoceptive  fibers 
to  cooling,  as  demonstrated  by  Hensel  &  Zotterman 
(53),  requires  a  sudden  temperature  rise  of  more  than 
8°C.  Since  the  cold  fibers  from  the  facial  region  of  the 
cat  which  have  been  more  closely  studied  (46)  do  not 
behave  differently  from  those  of  the  tongue,  there  can 
be  hardly  any  doubt  that  these  fibers  possess  endings 
which  are  specifically  stimulated  when  the  layer  of 
the  skin  in  which  thev  are  situated  is  cooled. 


Afferent  Nerve  Paths 

Judging  from  the  relative  spike  height  Zotterman 
(96)  suggested  that  the  cold  fibers  of  the  tongue  of  the 
cat  were  fairly  thin  myelinated  fibers  belonging  to  the 
6-group  of  the  class  A  fibers  according  to  Gasser  & 
Erlanger's  nomenclature  (fig.  10).  Direct  measure- 
ments of  isolated  single  cold  fibers  from  the  saphenous 
nerve  of  the  cat  by  Maruhashi  et  al.  (68)  gave  diam- 
eters of  1.5  to  3  ti.  These  fibers  showed  a  punctiform 
receptive  field  and  were  the  smallest  of  the  myelinated 
fibers.  They  were  sensitive  neither  to  light  touch  nor 
to  pinprick.  As  the  warm  fibers  give  rise  to  spikes  of 
somewhat  higher  amplitudes,  they  are  considered  to 
be  of  slightly  greater  diameters  (54,  96). 

The  central  course  of  the  temperature  fibers  in 
man  is  only  roughly  known.  After  entering  the  spinal 
cord  via  the  dorsal  roots  the  thermal  fibers  form  a 
lateral  division  which  enters  the  dorsolateral  fasciculus 
or  the  tract  of  Lissauer.  The  fibers  ascend  only  one  to 
three  segments  before  terminating  in  the  substantia 
gelatinosa  Rolandi,  a  cell  column  capping  the  poste- 
rior horn  with  a  .seemingly  uniform  texture  containing 
small  cell  bodies  only  and  with  no  large  myelinated 
fibers  traversing  it.  The  axons  of  its  small  cells  cross 
the  cord  in  the  anterior  gray  commissure  and  ascend 
in  the  lateral  spinothalamic  tract  (76).  In  syringo- 
myelia the  fibers  crossing  in  the  narrow  space  of  the 


anterior  gray  commissure  are  often  destroyed  which 
leads  to  a  well-known  clinical  syndrome  characterized 
by  loss  of  pain,  warmth  and  cold  on  both  sides  of  the 
body  at  the  level  of  the  segments  involved  while  the 
sense  of  touch  and  pressure  is  preserved.  In  some 
patients  there  may  be  a  dissociation  between  the 
degree  of  impairment  of  heat  and  cold  sensation. 

According  to  Haggqvist  (36)  as  well  as  to  Bailey  & 
Glees  (3)  the  majority  of  the  fibers  in  the  spinothalamic 
tract  are  2  to  4  /x  in  diameter,  35  per  cent  are  4  to  6  /i 
and  only  a  few  fibers  run  up  to  to  fi  in  diameter.  Thus 
the  dimensions  of  thermal  and  nociceptive  peripheral 
fibers  seem  to  be  preserved  in  the  second  order  of 
neurons.  The  spinothalamic  tract  is  .so  organized  that 
fibers  ascending  from  the  caudal  region  are  pushed 
outwards  by  the  accretion  of  crossing  fibers  at  each 
successive  segment  (93).  Fibers  from  the  cervical 
part  are  thus  situated  most  anterior  and  medially. 
This  arrangement  seems  to  maintain  the  topographi- 
cal organization  of  the  fibers  into  the  cortical  projec- 
tion. 

The  small-sized  temperature  fibers  of  the  trigeminal 
nerve  follow  the  course  of  the  pain  fibers  after  entering 
the  brain  stem  into  the  elongated  spinal  nucleus  which 
extends  through  the  medulla  to  meet  the  substantia 
gelatinosa  Rolandi  (28).  Division  of  this  tractus 
spinalis  of  the  trigeminal  nerve,  the  trigeminal  tractot- 
omy of  Sjoqvist,  in  the  medulla  leads  to  an  analgesia 
and  also  to  a  fairly  complete  thermal  anesthesia  in 
the  opposite  half  of  the  face  as  well  as  to  failure  of 
tickling  sensations  (79,  97).  The  exact  localization  of 
the  third  thermoceptive  neurons  in  the  thalamus  is 
not  known.  The  spinothalamic  tract  fiiaers  from  dififer- 
ent  levels  of  the  spinal  cord  terminate  in  the  postero- 
ventral  nucleus  of  the  thalamus  but  in  doing  so  they 
interdigitate  so  much  that  the  original  peripheral 
topography  of  fibers  mediating  different  modalities 
seems  to  be  regained.  In  the  ventral  nuclei  of  the 
thalamus  the  finer  topographical  organization  has 
been  worked  out  by  studying  degeneration  of  the 
fibers  in  the  medial  lemniscus  and  the  spinothalamic 
tract  (13)  but  a  still  more  detailed  map  was  obtained 
by  Mountcastle  &  Henneman  (69)  by  studying  the 
electric  response  appearing  in  the  thalamus  on  stimu- 
lation of  points  on  the  body  surface.  The  body  surface 
is  projected  onto  the  thalamus,  specifically  onto  the 
posteroventral  nucleus  which  is  the  only  part  in  which 
stimulation  of  the  skin  evoked  any  electric  response,  in 
such  a  way  that  the  head  is  represented  posteromedi- 
ally,  the  tail  anterolaterally,  the  back  superiorly  and 
the  feet  inferiorly.  According  to  Ruch  (76)  this 
topography  manifested  in  the  thalamic  terminations 


436 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOCV 


NEUROPHVSIOLOOV    I 


/\    "w   <i»>i«*»OMll*U^wi<WWWi>il»»«>»*»»>*» 


Q  iV^i^owt-iAt^vUiW^K^y^*^*^^^*^^"**'  ^M^MAyfif^'^*'^ 


>MAMMMMAMMAWM«MWWMnW^MWMW«WM*MWWMIWW(MM«inM^ 


ImV 


FIG.  2.  Cortical  cell  responding  specifically  to  cooling  of  the 
tong\ie  (cat).  A,  water  of  1 1  °C  and  B,  water  of  37  °C  were 
applied  to  the  tongue  (signal  on  lower  beam).  Time,  50  cps. 
C  and  D  show  the  same  cell  responding  to  electrical  stimulation 
of  the  tip  of  the  tongue.  .S',  stimulus  artifact.  Note  that  the 
first  spikes  do  not  appear  until  the  falling  phase  of  the  primary 
cortical  response.  Time,  5  msec.  Negativity  upwards  in  all 
records.  [From  Landgrcn,  S-,  personal  communication.] 


of  sensory  systems  is  preserved  in  the  thalamocortical 
projections.  The  medially  situated  arcuate  nucleus 
receiving  impulses  from  the  face  projects  near  the 
Sylvian  fissure.  The  lateral  part  of  the  posterolateral 
nucleus,  receiving  impulses  from  the  leg,  projects 
near  the  mid-line.  The  projection  from  the  arm  is 
intermediate  in  both  thalamus  and  cortex.  As  far  as 
anatomical  studies  have  revealed,  the  sensory  body 
surface  is  projected  upon  the  postcentral  gyrus  with 
spatial  relations  preserved  but  in  an  opposite  direction 
compared  with  that  in  the  thalamus.  Lacking  any 
direct  evidence  of  the  localization  of  the  third  thermal 
neuron  in  the  thalamus  we  have  to  suppose  that  the 
original  peripheral  topographical  organization  of  the 
thermal  units  is  regained  at  this  level. 

Electrical  stimulation  of  the  somesthetic  areas  of 
the  cortex  made  on  conscious  patients  (16,  71)  gives 
rise  to  localized  sensations.  The  most  usual  responses 
are  numbness,  tingling  and  a  feeling  of  movement 
and  only  more  rarely  warmth  and  cold  are  experi- 
enced. When  recording  from  single  cortical  cells  in 
the  cat  by  means  of  fine  microelectrodes,  Cohen  et  al. 
(15)  have  found  cells  in  the  tongue  sensory  area  which 
respond  specifically  to  cooling  of  the  tip  of  the  tongue 
but  not  to  mechanical  or  taste  stimuli.  Further  investi- 
gations by  Landgren  (63)  show  that  in  response  to 
cooling  of  the  tongue  cortical  cold  cells  produce  a 
discharge,  the  latency,  frequency  and  duration  of 
which  is  dependent  upon  the  strength  of  the  thermal 
stimulus.  The  shortest  recorded  latency  of  the  specific 


cortical  cold  cells  to  an  electric  .shock  to  the  tongue 
was  0.015  sec.  compared  to  0.005  ^ec.  of  a  cortical 
touch  cell  within  the  same  area.  The  shortest  latency 
recorded  to  cooling  of  the  tongue  was  about  0.02  sec. 
(fig.  2).  The  receptive  fields  of  the  specific  cold  cells 
were  limited  to  the  tip  or  the  lateral  edge  of  the 
tongue.  Besides  these  specific  cells  other  cells  were 
found  which  responded  to  mechanical  as  well  as  to 
thermal  stimuli,  occasionally  also  to  taste  stimuli. 
These  nonspecific  cortical  cells  showed  much  longer 
latencies  (0.08  to  0.30  sec.)  which  suggests  that  they 
cannot  be  primary.  So  far  only  one  cortical  cell 
responding  specifically  to  warming  the  tongue  was 
found.  It  thus  looks  as  though  the  thermosensitive 
imits  are  represented  in  the  cortex  topographically  in 
much  the  same  way  as  on  the  surface  of  the  body.  The 
fact  that  we  have  found  in  the  somesthetic  cortical 
areas  cells  which  respond  specifically  to  cold  or  to 
warmth  does  not  exclude  the  possibility  that  there  are 
peripheral  afferent  neurons  which  respond  to  thermal 
as  well  as  to  mechanical  and  noxious  stimuli.  Such 
neurons  can,  however,  scarcely  contribute  to  the 
specific  thermal  discrimination.  For  that  purpose  we 
have  to  reckon  with  the  activity  of  specific  peripheral 
neurons  finally  activating  specific  cortical  neurons. 
All  previous  speculation  of  a  possible  frequency  code 
is  not  only  incompatible  with  Johannes  Miiller's 
law  of  specific  sensory  energies  as  currently  conceived 
but  also  with  recent  electrophysiological  investiga- 
tions of  the  impulse  traffic  in  sensory  nerve  fibers. 
Although  many  have  looked  for  facts  indicating  some 
kind  of  frequency  code  there  is  to  date  very  little 
evidence  that  frequency  modulation  in  the  sensory 
nerve  can  influence  anything  but  the  intensity  of  the 
cortical  events  underlying  the  sensation.  This  opinion 
will  not  be  changed  if  nerve  fibers  are  found  with 
endings  which  are  not  strictK  functionally  specific. 
Some  of  the  unmvelinated  afferent  cutaneous  fibers 
are  most  probably  acti\ated  by  strong  abnormal 
stimulation  as  well  as  by  inechanical  stimulation. 
The  interference  of  these  fibers  with  the  activity 
of  more  strictly  specific  fibers  inay  very  well  under- 
lie such  cutaneous  sensations  as  hot  and  tickling 
which  possess  something  more  than  one  sensory 
quality.  No  nerve  endings  are  aijsolutely  specific  as 
they  are  all  excitable  by  electrical  stimulation  or  by 
strong  mechanical  or  chemical  stimulation.  Thus 
when  we  speak  of  specific  nerve  endings  from  a  func- 
tional point  of  view,  we  refer  only  to  such  sensory 
end  organs  as  are  specific  within  reasonable  limits. 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


437 


CONDITIONS  FOR  THERMAL  SENSATIONS 

The  variety  of  opinion  concerning  the  conditions 
for  thermal  stimulation,  which  until  lately  has  charac- 
terized the  discussion  of  the  temperature  senses  ever 
since  Weber  presented  his  famous  theory  in  1 846,  was 
to  a  great  extent  dependent  upon  imperfections  in  the 
physical  methods  used  in  studying  the  thermal  move- 
ments in  the  skin  as  well  as  upon  the  use  of  subjective 
reports  as  an  indicator  of  the  stimulating  eflTcct.  The 
main  problems  in  the  physiology  of  the  thermal  senses 
have  been  the  question  of  whether  temporal  tempera- 
ture changes  or  the  absolute  temperature  levels  were 
the  adequate  stimulus  and  the  intimately  connected 
question  of  the  physical  or  physiological  interpreta- 
tion of  adaptation. 


Conductiiin  of  Heal  in  Skin 

While  the  majority  of  writers  have  on  the  whole 
accepted  Weber's  opinion  that  the  temporal  diflfer- 
ential  quotient  of  the  temperature  change  represents 
the  adequate  stimulus,  there  are  others  who  like 
Hering  (58)  have  given  attention  to  the  influence  of 
the  prevailing  temperature  in  itself  (37).  Thunberg 
stated  in  1905  in  Nagel's  Handhucli  that  this  question 
cannot  be  settled  until  the  physical  constants  of  the 
external  layers  of  the  skin  are  so  well  known  that  the 
thermal  exchange  in  the  skin  can  be  computed  quanti- 
tatively. Following  the  work  of  Bazett  et  al.  (5), 
Hensel  (42,  43)  succeeded  in  developing  methods 
for  the  determination  of  intracutaneous  temperature 
at  exactly  localized  depths,  a  very  fine  thermocouple 
being  introduced  through  a  thin  cannula  or  through 
an  intracutaneous  punctured  channel. 

Further,  Hensel  (43)  constructed  a  precision-flow- 
calorimeter  for  measuring  the  steady  heat  flow  given 
oflf  from  small  skin  areas.  The  Stromungskalorim- 
eter — a  flat  cylindrical  measuring  chamber  through 
which  water  of  constant  temperature  flows  with  con- 
stant velocity — is  placed  on  the  skin  above  the  two 
thermocouples  which  are  situated  at  different  depths. 
The  amount  of  heat  given  off  is  then  obtained  from 
the  flow  velocity  and  temperature  difference  between 
inflowing  and  outflowing  water.  The  mean  error  of 
the  method  is  as  low  as  about  ±0.001  cal.  per  cm- 
per  sec. 

The  thermal  movement  in  nonstationary  conditions 
depends  not  only  upon  the  thermal  conductivity  of 
the  tissue  but  also  on  its  specific  heat  and  density. 


The    determining    constant,    the    thermal    diffusion 
coefficient,  a,  is  obtained  by  the  following  equation : 


where  X  (calories  per  cm  per  sec.  per  degree)  repre- 
sents the  thermal  conductivity,  C  (cal.  per  gm  per 
degree)  the  specific  heat,  and  p  (gm  per  cm-*)  the 
density  of  the  substance.  As  the  determination  of  C 
and  X  are  problematic  in  the  living  skin,  Hensel  C44) 
elaborated  a  method  for  direct  determination  of  the 
diffusivity,  a.  By  means  of  the  above  described  thermo- 
electrical  methods  the  temperature  movements  were 
recorded  at  diflferent  depths  of  the  skin  when  rectangu- 
lar temperature  pulses  were  applied  to  the  surface  by 
the  application  of  metal  bodies  of  constant  tempera- 
ture. From  the  curves  obtained  the  diffusivity,  a, 
could  be  determined  in  that  the  curves  for  various 
values  of  a  were  constructed  and  it  was  found  at 
which  value  of  a  the  computed  curve  best  fitted  the 
recorded  curve.  For  human  skin  in  depths  up  to  2 
mm   the  values  for  a  varied   from  0.0004  to  0.0018 


32 


M 


ss 


a, 
S 


21 


^com'/iK  —^fioiiiiK.-^joae'hK.  - 


» 

Time 


-VlvK 


ill''.' 


fimse 


FIG.  3.  Recorded  intracutaneous  temperature  change  at  a 
depth  of  0.6  mm  on  application  of  a  thermode  at  I7°C  on  the 
skin  at  33.5^0.  A  distinct  cold  sensation  persisted  throughout 
the  whole  experiment  although  the  rate  of  change  after  3  rnin. 
fell  below  the  minimum  value  of  o.oo25°C  per  sec.  given  by 
Gertz  for  the  maintenance  of  a  cold  sensation.  [From  Hensel 
(42)-] 


438 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  --'   NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


30 

Temperature     G 


FIG.  4.  'Adaptation  periods'  (the  time  until  the  temperature  sensation  disappears)  as  a  function 
of  the  stimulus  teinperature  when  using  constant  temperature  for  stimulation.  The  adaptation 
periods  become  longer  as  the  stimulus  temperature  departs  from  the  indifferent  temperature 
(32.5''C).  Broken  lines  indicate  periods  as  function  of  the  stimulus  temperature  after  which  the  intra- 
cutaneous temperature  change  at  a  depth  of  i  mm  has  fallen  below  values  of  0.2  "C  per  sec.  and  of 
o.o°C  per  sec.  The  adaptation  periods  do  not  at  all  coincide  with  the  subsidence  of  the  intracuta- 
neous temperature  change.  [From  Hensel  (42).] 


cm-  per  sec.  according  to  depth  of  the  layer  and  the 
cutaneous  circulation. 

By  theoretical  as  well  as  by  experimental  investiga- 
tion Hensel  (44)  showed  that  changes  in  the  blood 
flow  in  the  skin  exerts  much  less  influence  upon  the 
diff'usivity  (the  thermometric  conducti\ity)  than  it 
does  upon  the  thermal  conductivity. 

Relation  Between  Temperature  Change  Recorded  in 
Skin  and  Thermal  Sensations 

Using  the  methods  descrilsed  above  Hensel  (43,  44) 
made  thorough  investigations  of  the  relation  between 
the  thermal  sensations  reported  by  the  subject  and  the 
actual  temperature  movements  in  the  skin  when  well 
defined  thermal  stimulation  was  applied  to  the  skin. 

In  confirmation  of  earlier  workers,  Heilbrun  (41) 
and  Hensel  (42)  demonstrated  that  thermal  sensa- 
tions still  persisted  when  the  temperature  of  the  skin 
had  reached  a  constant  level.  With  the  above  de- 
scriljed  method  Hensel  recorded  the  temperature 
movements  at  a  depth  of  0.6  mm  when  a  rectangular 


thermal  step,  t,  was  applied.  As  will  he  seen  in  figure 
3,  the  rate  of  thermal  change  had  gone  down  below 
the  value  of  0.0025°  P^r  ^^c.  which  Gertz  (30)  had 
found  to  be  the  minimum  rate  necessary  to  maintain 
a  thermal  sensation,  .\fter  20  min.,  when  the  tempera- 
ture had  been  practically  constant  for  some  minutes, 
there  was  still  reported  a  diminishing  but  quite  dis- 
tinct cold  sensation. 

In  figure  4  the  adaptation  period  (interval  from 
the  stimulus  application  until  the  disappearance  of 
thermal  sensation)  and  the  interval  until  the  tempera- 
ture change  stopped  is  plotted  against  the  tempera- 
ture applied  to  the  skin.  At  temperatures  below  20°C 
and  above  40°C  constant  sensations  appear.  Hensel 
(42)  found  that  the  adaptation  requires  a  longer  time 
the  more  the  temperature  of  the  stimulus  diverges 
from  the  temperature  of  the  skin.  But  the  cessation  of 
the  thermal  sensation  and  the  intracutaneous  tempera- 
ture changes  do  not  coincide,  as  the  sensation  usually 
considerably  outlasts  the  intracutaneous  temperature 
movement.  This  is  particularly  the  case  at  extreme 
temperatures. 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


439 


K/iK 

0,03 


0.0! 


0.01 


J L 


^T . 


32  33  30  35  36  37  3S 

Temperature    6 


—is- 


»C  ft 


FIG.  5.  Rate,  d^/d/,  of  the  intracutaneous  temperature  change 
at  the  subsidence  of  the  warm  sensation  as  a  function  of  the 
prevalent  temperature.  (Forearm,  skin  temperature  3 1  °C, 
thermode  area  of  20  cm^.)  It  will  be  seen  that  smaller  values  of 
d9/d<  are  necessary  for  maintaining  a  warm  sensation,  the 
higher  the  prevailing  temperature.  [From  Hensel  C4J).j 


The  rate  (d0/d/)  of  the  intracutaneous  temperature 
change  at  the  moment  of  the  subsidence  of  the  warm 
sensation  is  shown  in  figure  5.  Thus,  the  rate  of  temp- 
erature change  beUeved  necessary  to  maintain  a  sen- 
sation diminishes  the  more  extreme  the  temperature 
until  at  certain  threshold  temperatures  it  attains  the 
value  of  o.  Outside  this  threshold  value  a  constant 
temperature  acts  as  a  stimulus  eliciting  a  steady 
thermal  sensation. 


The  experiments  of  Gertz  (30)  on  the  effect  of 
approximately  linear  changes  of  temperature  have 
been  repeated  using  more  accurate  methods  by 
Hensel  (42).  As  will  be  seen  from  figure  6,  the  thermal 
sensations  pass  successively  through  all  grades  of  sen- 
sation from  cold  to  warmth,  although  the  rate  of 
change  (d9/dO  is  kept  constant.  If  at  any  stage  of  the 
procedure  the  temperature  change  is  allowed  to  stop 
(d9/d<  =  o),  the  thermal  sensation  in  question  at 
once  becomes  definitely  weaker. 

With  uniform  rates  of  change  of  diff'erent  slope  the 
time  factor  (adaptation)  will  produce  a  shift  of  the 
threshold  in  such  a  way  that  the  slower  the  rate  of 
change  the  more  will  the  sensory  threshold  be  trans- 
ferred to  the  extreme  regions  of  temperature.  A 
typical  experiment  is  illustrated  in  figure  7  from 
which  it  must  be  concluded  that  in  the  determination 
of  the  thresholds  for  warmth  and  cold  there  must 
exist  a  mutual  relationship  between  two  factors:  the 
prevalent  temperature  and  the  temporal  slope  (AQjAl) 
of  the  intracutaneous  temperature  change. 

Figure  8  gives  a  graphical  description  of  the  mutual 
relationship  between  the  prevalent  temperature  and 
the  temporal  differential  quotient,  d9  d/,  in  relation 
to  thermal  .sensations.  The  points  of  the  curves  repre- 
sent average  values  from  a  great  number  of  experi- 
ments. As  will  be  seen,  temperature  changes  of 
-|-o.ooi°  per  sec.  and  —0.001°  per  sec.  are  still  effec- 
tive at  temperatures  above  38°  and  below  25°.  Out- 


u 


i2 


I- 
< 

a:     so 

u 

a. 

2 

u 

^     28 


26 


LUKEWARM 


+  0.  45     C/MIN 


LUKEWARM 

INDIFFERENT 
COOL 
COLD 


.INDIFFERENT 


COOL 


0.87  °C/MIN 


COLD 


I  .  I  .  I  ■  I  ■  I  ■  '  ■  ' '  ■ 1 


10  20  30  40  50  60  70  80  90  100     MIN 

TIME      t 


FIG.  6.  Course  of  the  temperature  sensation  at  rectilinear  warming  and  cooling  of  the  foot  in  an 
ultrathermostate  according  to  Hoppler.  [From  Hensel  (45).] 


440  HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV   ^  NEUROPHVSIOLOCY    1 


f-C^OOSJ'/set: 


'■0,02°l%iz 


—    Thermode  temperature 
Skin  temperature    0.  5mm 


10  min   11 


Time    t 


FIG.  7  Position  of  the  warmtli  and  cold  thresholds  on  the  forearm  at  rectilinear  temperature 
changes  of  different  directions  and  slopes  (thermode  area  of  20  cm-).  The  slower  the  rate  of  change, 
the  more  distant  from  the  indifferent  temperature  (33.4°C)  the  thresholds  lie.  [From  Hensel  (42).] 


Zi 


29 


30 


31         32         33         3¥         35 
Temperature    9 


37 


31  "C  33 


FIG.  8-  Position  of  warm  and  cold  thresholds  in  relation  to  the  rate  of  temperature  change,  dfl/d/, 
and  the  temperature  9  of  the  skin.  Broken  lines,  threshold  sensation;  solid  line,  distinct  sensation.  Initial 
temperature  in  all  experiments,  33.3°C.  [From  Hensel  (42)-] 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


441 


side  this  temperature  region  the  required  slope 
dd/dt  sinks  further  until  it  finally  attains  a  zero  value, 
i.e.  where  the  temperature  level  itself  is  sufficient  for 
elicitation  of  the  sensation. 

When  the  temperature  change  starts  from  different 
temperature  levels  (adaptation  temperature),  the 
thresholds  for  the  warm  .sensation  will  reach  different 
values  (see  fig.  9).  With  a  constant  rate  of  change  of 
0.0017°  P^i"  sec.  it  is  thus  found  that  the  threshold  for 
warmth  will  depend  upon  the  initial  temperature  to 
which  the  receptors  have  been  adapted.  The  lower 
this  initial  temperature,  the  greater  the  heating  has 
to  be  in  order  to  elicit  a  sensation  of  warmth.  For  the 
cold  sensation  it  is  the  other  way  when  initial  tem- 
perature is  lowered.  Here  the  cooling  necessary 
becomes  less  and  less  intense  until  the  temperature 
region  is  reached  where  a  steady  cold  sensation  ensues. 
For  higher  initial  temperatures,  the  opposite  holds. 
Here  the  cold  receptors  for  equal  cold  steps  become 
less  sensitive  the  higher  the  initial  temperature  is 
taken  (23,  37,  42). 

It  has  long  been  recognized  that  the  stimulated 
area  and  thus  the  number  of  stimulated  thermal  re- 
ceptors must  be  of  great  importance  in  the  production 


t-i 

U 
(U 

£ 

H 


37 

"C 
3f 

35 

3V 

33 

32 

3! 

30 

29 

26 

2S 
25 


W* 


w* 


W+  warm  threshold 
W++  distinct  warmth 


Time  t 


J L 


J 


8       min  70 


FIG.  9.  Warm  thresholds  on  forearm  exposed  to  a  rectilinear 
increase  of  temperature  of  o.oi7°C  per  sec.  from  initial  tempera- 
tures of  25°,  30°  and  35°C.  (Thermode  area,  20  cm^.)  [From 
Hensel  (45).] 


of  thermal  sensations,  although  for  long  periods  the 
use  of  more  or  less  punctiform  stimuli  has  been  preva- 
lent. The  temperature  sense  in  life  situations  is  affected 
o\er  much  of  the  body  surface  as  Hensel  (45)  em- 
phasizes. This  is  inter  alia  seen  from  the  fact  that  the 
cold  and  warm  spots  were  not  discovered  until  modern 
times  (9),  although  they  can  be  detected  by  the  most 
simple  devices. 

Investigations  on  the  temperature  sensations  when 
the  whole  body  surface  was  exposed  have  been  made 
by  Marechaux  &  Schafcr  (67).  In  a  climate  chamber 
of  the  type  used  by  Wetzler  &  Thauer  the  subjects 
were  exposed  to  approximately  linear  increases  of  the 
temperature  with  a  slope  of  o.ooi  to  o.oi°C  per  sec. 
As  the  temperature  of  the  chamber  rose,  the  skin 
temperature  of  the  different  parts  of  the  body  rose 
relatively  linearly.  The  average  rate  of  the  skin  tem- 
perature rise  during  the  most  rapid  rises  amounted 
to  0.0015  to  0.003°  per  sec.  and  during  the  slowest 
rise  to  less  than  o.ooi  °  per  sec.  Starting  from  a  general 
coolish  sensation  the  sensation  of  warmth  appeared 
regularly  in  the  following  order:  forehead-abdomen- 
hand-foot,  in  agreement  with  the  investigations  on 
more  limited  areas  carried  out  i)y  Gilsbach  (31)  and 
Hensel  (42).  The  sen.sations  produced  appeared  in  this 
order:  cold-indifTerent-faintly  warm-distinctly  warm. 
Table  2  shows  the  warmth  threshold  temperature 
with  slow  rise  of  the  chamber  temperature.  As  will 
be  seen  it  is  not  possible  even  with  an  extremely  slow 
rate  of  temperature  rise  at  less  than  0.001°  per  sec. 
to  avoid  the  production  of  a  sensation  of  warmth  when 
the  temperature  of  the  skin  is  aijove  35°C.  The  region 
of  thermal  indifference  when  the  whole  body  is  con- 
cerned is  thus  limited  to  a  small  region  between  about 
32  to  35°C.  Previous  investigations  of  Rein  &  Strug- 
hold  (73,  74),  Stein  &  von  Weizsacker  (82),  Bohnen- 
kamp  &  Pasquai  (11),  Hardy  &  Oppel  (38)  and 
Herget  et  al.  (57)  have  all  shown  that  there  is  a  marked 
decline  in  the  threshold  when  the  number  of  stimu- 
lated sen.sory  spots  is  increased. 

Besides  the  temporal  temperature  gradient  (dd/dt) 
a  spatial  temperature  gradient  (d^/d*)  has  been 
widely  discussed.  Ebbecke  (26)  observed  that  the 
release  of  blood  flow  into  a  previously  clamped  and 
cooled  limb  elicited  an  intense  and  unexpected  .sensa- 
tion of  cold.  This  led  him  to  suggest  that  a  cold  sensa- 
tion is  produced  by  a  temperature  difference  in  the 
skin  at  the  border  line  of  the  epidermis  and  the  cutis, 
while  a  warmth  sensation  is  produced  by  a  tempera- 
ture difference  at  the  border  line  between  the  cutis 
and  the  subcutis,  the  direction  of  the  temperature 
gradient  being  immaterial.   This  idea  was  confuted 


442  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 

TABLE  2.   Threshold  ']  emperatiiri's  Jul    Warmth  During  Healing  of  Body  in  Climate  Chamber* 


Rate  of  Increase 
degrees/sec. 

Forehead  °C 

Abdomen  °C 

Hand  °C 

Foot  -C 

Integrated  Skin  Temp.  °C 

O.OOI 
0,002-0.003 

34.8±o.3 

34-7±o.5 

34-5±o.6 
34.8±o.8 

3i-7±i-> 
3'-5±i-9 

3i-5±i-i 
3i-5±i.9 

34.2±o.6 
34-3±o-94 

*  Average  values  from  8  experiments    [From  Marechaux  &  Schafer  (67).] 


by  Goldscheider  &  Hahn  (33)  who  showed  that  .sub- 
cutaneous injections  of  saline  of  I3°C  ehcited  a  dis- 
tinct cold  sensation  while  injections  of  50°C  saline 
elicited  a  sensation  of  warmth,  whereas  according  to 
Ebbecke's  view  a  sensation  of  warmth  should  have 
been  produced  in  both  cases. 

From  further  experiments  in  which  the  tempera- 
ture change  and  the  change  of  the  spatial  gradient 
ran  in  opposite  directions,  Hensel  (42)  concluded  that 
the  intracutaneous  temperature  gradient  or  its  tem- 
poral change  cannot  be  the  decisive  condition  for  the 
production  of  a  thermal  sensation  but  the  simple 
warming  or  cooling  of  the  receptors,  independent  of 
the  intracutaneous  temperature  gradient,  is  deter- 
minative. Bazett  &  McGlone  (4)  also  observed  that 
cooling  from  the  lower  surface  of  a  double  fold  of  the 
skin  of  the  prepuce  led  to  a  sensation  of  cold  in  the 
upper  surface.  In  their  further  investigations,  carried 
out  to  test  the  validity  of  Ebbecke's  spatial  gradient 
hypothesis,  they  observed  that  sensations  of  intense 
warmth  or  heat  were  found  to  be  induced  on  release 
of  stasis  in  a  limb  maintained  before,  during  and  after 
stasis  in  a  bath  at  the  blood  temperature  level,  so  that 
on  release  no  changes  in  temperature  occurred  and 
no  thermal  gradient  was  established.  This  warm 
sensation  they  attributed  to  a  chemical  stimulus 
derived  from  metabolic  processes  particularlv  in 
muscle  tissue  by  means  of  a  substance  that  varies  in 
concentration  both  during  asphyxia  and  as  a  result 
of  temperature  changes  in  a  manner  similar  to  that 
of  acid. 

According  to  Lewis  et  al.  (66)  and  Zotterman  (95) 
the  sensation  of  tingling  which  occurs  after  the  release 
of  the  blood  flow  to  limbs  in  which  the  circulation  has 
been  arrested  is  attributable  to  an  excitation  of  fibers 
in  the  nerve  trunk  in  the  area  of  compression.  The 
sensation  of  tingling  after  release  of  the  blood  flow 
can  be  greatly  enhanced  by  hyperventilation  and  its 
appearance  can  be  entirely  prevented  by  breathing 
1 2  per  cent  carbon  dioxide  in  oxygen  (29).  The  sensa- 
tion of  warmth  upon  release  has,  however,  a  quite 
different  time  course  from  that  of  tingling  which 
appears  after  a  latency  of  30  to  60  sec,  and  the  sensa- 


tion of  warmth  which  is  immediately  experienced 
upon  release  is  therefore  most  likely  due  to  a  stimula- 
tion of  the  receptors  in  the  periphery,  as  Bazett  & 
McGlone  assumed.  The  only  direct  knowledge  of  the 
influence  of  ischemia  on  thermal  receptors  comes 
from  Hensel  (47)  but  is  limited  to  the  behavior  of  cold 
receptors.  He  noticed  that  ischemia  abolished  within 
a  few  minutes  the  steady  discharge  of  the  cold  fibers. 
Upon  release  of  the  blood  flow  the  discharge  immedi- 
ately reappeared  reaching  the  initial  frequency  within 
15  to  30  sec.  In  the  same  way  as  the  steady  discharge 
disappears  during  ischemia,  the  excitability  of  the 
receptor  to  cold  increases  is  gradually  paralyzed. 

Although  Lele  et  al.  (65)  repudiate  "the  spatial 
intracutaneous  gradient  theory  which  is  based  upon 
the  assumed  presence  of  specific  encapsulated  thermal 
receptors"  on  anatomical  as  well  as  on  physiological 
grounds,  they  maintain  that  the  thermal  sensations 
reported  in  the  presence  of  an  absolutely  constant 
surface  skin  temperature  are  due  to  a  difference  in 
temperature  between  different  strata  of  the  skin  in 
which  the  terminals  of  the  unencapsulated  nerve 
endings  and  their  nerve  trunk  lie.  They  suggest  that 
the  unencapsulated  endings  give  rise  to  propagated 
disturbances  when  a  difference  of  temperature  exists 
between  stem  axons  and  terminals  and  that  they  are 
so  arranged  that  the  skin  l)ehaves  as  a  thermopile 
type  of  'bolometer'  rather  than  as  'thermometer'. 
They  believe  that  the  anatomical  arrangement  of 
these  unencapsulated  nerve  terminals  in  the  skin  is 
such  that  it  is  likely  that  different  teinporospatial 
patterns  of  action  potentials  will  be  evoked  from  the 
same  area  of  skin  when  the  temperature  is  raised  or 
lowered.  The  patterns  evoked  will  not  be  due  to  the 
fact  that  certain  receptors  have  specific  properties  not 
possessed  by  others  but  due  to  the  fact  that  numerous 
nonspecific  receptors  are  disposed  in  different  strata 
of  the  skin  which  are  not  at  the  same  temperature. 
They  further  maintain  that  these  endings,  which  sub- 
serve warm  and  cold  sensibility  can,  if  stimulated  in 
the  appropriate  way,  give  rise  to  other  sensations  not 
associated  with  the  thermal  modality  such  as  touch, 
prick,  itch  and  sharp  pain. 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS  443 


This  opinion  is  quite  incompatible  with  the  electro- 
physiological findings  of  Zotterman  (96),  Hensel  & 
Zotterman  (52,  54)  and  Hensel  (45,  46)  that  cold 
and  warmth  are  subserved  by  specific  peripheral 
neurons  which  are  relatively  inexcitable  by  mechani- 
cal stimuli.  Hensel's  thermoelectrical  recordings  show- 
that  a  metal  thermode  through  which  water  flows 
quickly  dominates  entirely  the  temperature  condi- 
tions in  the  superior  layers  of  the  skin.  The  blood 
temperature  has  little  or  no  effect.  At  a  constant 
thermode  temperature  at  3o°C  or  above,  no  appreci- 
able temperature  change  was  observed  in  the  layer  of 
the  cold  receptors  in  the  tongue  when  the  blood  flow- 
was  arrested  or  released.  The  release  of  the  blood  flow 
to  the  tongue  which  previously  had  been  ischemic  for 
some  minutes  gave  rise  to  an  immediate  return  and 
enhancement  of  the  steady  discharge  from  the  cold 
receptors  previously  paralyzed  by  the  ischemia.  The 
Ebbecke  phenomenon  can  thus  not  be  explained  by 
thermal  changes  but  by  chemical  changes  induced  by 
the  ischemia.  The  effects  of  ischemia  occur  equally  at 
all  temperatures  between  20  and  32  °C  and  afso  when 
there  is  no  thermal  effect  of  the  blood  flow.  They 
must  all  be  due  to  oxygen  lack  (47). 

Paradoxical  Sensations 

Striimpell  (86)  described  patients  with  neurological 
diseases  displaying  specific  anesthesia  to  cold  and 
reported  a  very  distinct  heat  sensation  when  the  skin 
was  touched  by  pieces  of  ice.  The  reverse  was  less 
often  found,  i.e.  that  heating  the  skin  produced  a 
sensation  of  cold.  In  1895  von  Frey  (91)  definitely 
established  that  the  stimulation  of  single  cold  spots 
with  heat  above  45°C  caused  a  sensation  of  cold  which 
he  named  'paradoxical  cold  sensation.'  The  existence 
of  a  paradoxical  cold  sensation  has  been  generally 
accepted,  while  the  corresponding  paradoxical  warm 
sensation  still  is  under  debate.  Lehmann  (64),  Al- 
rutz  (i)  and  later  Rein  (72)  failed  to  produce  any 
paradoxical  sensation  of  warmth.  Thunberg  (8g) 
suggested  in  1905  that  this  most  likely  is  caused  by 
the  fact  that  the  intensive  cooling  evokes  a  very  in- 
tense cold  sensation  which  masks  the  paradoxical 
sensation  of  warmth  which  in  Striimpell's  case  of  cold 
anesthesia  was  obtained  unmasked.  Recent  electro- 
phy.siological  studies  (22)  reveal  that  warm  fibers 
actually  respond  to  rapid  cooling  of  8  to  i5°C  but 
this  has  more  the  character  of  an  off  discharge  of  a 
phasic  nature  since  it  soon  fades  away.  This  behavior 
of  the  warm  receptors  or  the  peripheral  parts  of  the 
warm    fiber   endings   explains   why   this   paradoxical 


discharge  of  warmth  is  more  difficult  to  detect  (cf. 
page  44B). 

7  liermal  After-Sensations 

Weber  (94)  had  great  difficult\-  in  interpreting  the 
phenomenon  of  the  'persisting  cold  sensation'  experi- 
enced for  instance  when  a  cold  metal  object  which  has 
been  pressed  for  about  half  a  minute  against  the  skin 
of  the  forehead  is  removed.  In  this  famous  experiment 
a  cold  sensation  is  thus  experienced  while  the  tempera- 
ture of  the  receptor  layer  of  the  skin  is  gradually 
warming  which  according  to  Weber's  theory  should 
lead  to  a  sensation  of  warmth.  Weber  himself  sug- 
gested that  this  cold  .sensation  was  due  to  a  further 
spread  of  the  cooling  to  surrounding  parts  of  the  skin, 
a  view  which  had  been  already  rejected  by  Hering 
(58)  because  of  the  inadequate  spread  of  the  cooling 
compared  to  the  marked  rewarming  of  the  cooled 
area.  Alrutz  (2)  and  Holm  (60)  suggested  that  the 
persisting  cold  sensation  was  due  to  paradoxical 
stimulation  of  the  cold  receptors  by  their  sudden 
rewarming  by  the  blood.  The  interpretation  of 
Weber  was  again  refuted  by  Holm  (60)  who  anes- 
thetized the  cooled  area  of  the  skin  leaving  the  sur- 
rounding area  intact.  In  spite  of  normal  thermal 
sensibility  in  the  surrounding  zone,  no  sensation  of 
cold  appeared.  Further,  Bazett  &  McGlone  (4)  re- 
corded the  .skin  temperature  below  the  cooled  area 
and  proved  that  the  cold  after-sensation  coincided  with 
an  actual  rewarming  of  the  skin  although  they  believed 
as  Weber  that  in  their  case  the  sensation  could  be 
attributed  to  a  spread  of  the  cooling  to  the  surround- 
ing skin. 

More  recently  Hensel  (42)  has  recorded  the  actual 
course  of  the  intracutaneous  temperature  movement 
below  as  well  as  outside  the  thermode.  He  demon- 
strated that  the  spread  of  cooling  to  adjacent  parts  of 
the  skin  is  very  slight,  the  quantitative  relation  be- 
tween the  rewarming  of  the  cooled  area  and  the  cool- 
ing of  the  surrounding  being  18:1  at  the  time  of  the 
most  intensive  cold  after-sensation. 

Thus  the  cold  after-sensation  cannot  be  explained 
by  a  subsequent  spread  of  cooling.  At  low  skin  tem- 
peratures a  cold  sensation  can  be  present  even  when 
the  temperature  of  the  .skin  is  gradually  rising.  This 
cold  sensation  is  just  a  normal  cold  sensation  due  to 
the  low  temperature  of  the  receptor  layer  of  the  skin. 
Electrophysiological  studies  of  the  activity  of  the  cold 
fibers  in  the  cat  (cf.  page  446)  very  substantially 
supports  the  view  that  the  cold  receptors  at  low  tem- 
peratures  are   displaying   a   steady   discharge  which 


444 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


increases  in  frequency  as  the  temperature  slowly  rises 
from  15  to  '25°C. 

Sensation  iij  'Hot' 

Alrutz  (2)  suggested  that  the  sensation  of  hot  was  a 
mixed  sensation  of  warmth  and  'paradoxical'  cold 
although  it  is  a  subjectively  simple  sensation  not 
divisible  by  introspective  analysis  and  is  qualitatixely 
different  from  the  sensations  of  cold  and  warmth. 
Thunberg  (87),  Kiesow  (62)  as  well  as  Trotter  & 
Davies  (90)  criticized  this  theory  of  Alrutz,  main- 
taining that  the  paradoxical  cold  sensation  can 
readily  be  apprehended  and  that  the  applied  heat 
even  stimulates  other  sensory  fibers  in  the  skin.  Hacker 
(35)  observed,  however,  in  an  experiment  on  him.self 
in  a  traumatized  region  of  the  skin  where  no  cold 
spots  but  numerous  warm  spots  were  found  that  no 
sensation  of  hotness  was  obtained  but  only  of  warmth. 
Goldscheider  (32),  however,  rejected  Alrutz'  inter- 
pretation because  the  sensation  of  hot  is  felt  most 
strongly  in  regions  where  the  warmth  sensibility 
is  particularly  good  and  not  in  regions  where  the 
cold  sensibility  is  comparatively  stronger  than  that 
of  warmth. 

Kaila  (61)  described  an  experiment  on  thermal 
receptors  of  the  penis  in  man  which  greatly  strengthens 
the  original  view  of  Alrutz  (2).  Usually  the  tip  of  the 
penis  does  not  possess  any  sensibility  of  warmth  while 
cold  and  pain  is  easily  evoked.  When  the  tip  of  the 
penis  is  dipped  into  water  of  40°C,  the  subject  experi- 
ences a  rather  unplea.sant  painful  sensation;  this 
temperature  does  not  act  on  the  cold  receptors.  If, 
however,  the  temperature  is  raised  to  45°C,  an  intense 
sensation  of  cold  is  produced  as  this  temperature 
stimulates  the  cold  fibers.  The  sensation  is,  however, 
not  really  painful.  When  now  a  greater  part  of  the 
penis  is  dipped  into  the  water  at  45 °C,  warm  recep- 
tors are  also  stimulated  and  a  specific  .sensation  of 
pleasant  heat  appears. 

This  simple  experiment  is  an  example  showing 
how  simultaneous  stimulation  of  different  receptors 
can  evoke  the  sensation  of  a  specific  quality  in  which 
it  is  not  possible  to  recognize  the  elementary  sensa- 
tions which  each  of  the  specific  receptors  involved 
produce.  Thus,  when  we  speak  of  'hot'  as  an  elemen- 
tary sen.sation,  as  Alrutz  did,  or  as  a  'fusion'  the  dis- 
tinction is  fictitious  because  the  integration  ma\'  ise 
effected  deeply  below  the  'threshold  of  consciousness." 
Head  (40)  maintained  that  this  occurred  as  early  as 
the  first  synapse.  For  further  analysis  see  page  452. 


ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY  OF  THERM.\L  NERVE  FIBERS 

In  recent  years  the  function  of  the  thermal  recep- 
tors has  been  subjected  to  more  objective  investigation 
by  the  comijination  of  effective  methods  for  recording 
the  temperature  and  the  action  potentials  from  the 
nerve  fibers  involved. 


Specificity  of  .Nerve  Fibers  in  Mammals 

The  first  recording  of  the  specific  electric  response 
to  thermal  stimulation  was  made  by  this  writer  in 
1936.  He  was  generally  able  to  see  leading  off  from  a 
fine  branch  of  the  lingual  nerve  a  number  of  small 
action  potentials  with  spike  heights  I3  to  ifo  of  that 
of  the  largest  spike  potentials  elicited  by  touching  the 
tongue  (fig.  10).  When  the  tongue  is  washed  with 
warm  water  these  small  spikes  disappear  to  return 
shortly  if  the  tongue  is  laid  free  in  the  air  at  room 
temperature.  A  faint  draft  over  the  tongue  increases 
the  number  of  impulses,  and  a  sudden  fine  stream  of 
air  from  a  syringe  on  the  receptive  field  elicits  a  dis- 


■■^W^"**^*V*^*^^<^ 


FIG.  10.  ^4.  Microphotograph  of  lingual  nerve  preparation. 
Magnification,  685.  Largest  fibers  measure  10  ^  in  diameter. 
.\lsheimer-Mann  stain.  B.  Record  from  the  same  preparation 
showing  the  ratio  between  the  spike  heights  of  cold  and  touch 
impulses.  The  irregular  discharge  of  the  small  cold  spikes  is 
due  to  the  exposure  of  the  tongue  to  air.  The  four  large  spikes 
were  elicited  by  touching  the  tongue  with  a  brush.  [From  Zot- 
terman  (96).] 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


445 


tinct  volley  of  small  spikes.  When  the  current  of  air  is 
forceful  enough  to  make  a  noticeable  deformation  on 
the  tongue,  larger  spikes  appear  among  an  increased 
number  of  small  ones  (fig.  1 1  B').  If  the  air  in  the  syringe 
is  successively  warmed,  a  point  is  reached  when  the 
air  stream  does  not  elicit  any  small  spikes,  while  the 
large  ones  still  appear  as  soon  as  the  pressure  is  raised 
sufficiently  to  occasion  a  noticeable  deformation  on 
the  surface  of  the  tongue. 

When  one  drop  of  hot  water  (8o°C)  is  applied  to 
the  tongue,  two  types  of  spikes  may  be  observed  be- 
sides the  large  spikes  signalling  the  impact  of  the  water 
drop  (fig.  11^).  A  careful  examination  of  the  record 
reveals  two  types  of  spikes  of  which  one  derives  from 
warm  fibers  and  the  other,  a  somewhat  smaller  and 
apparently  more  slowly  conducted  spike,  derives  from 
a  pain  fiber.  In  this  way  it  was  possible  to  show  that 
cold  and  warmth  as  well  as  pain  are  mediated  in 
specific  nerve  fibers  (96). 

Thermal  Receptors  in  Cold-Blooded  Animals 

Electrophysiological  investigations  by  Sand  (77) 
on  single  fibers  from  the  Lorenzinian  ampullae  of 
Raja  showed  that  the  receptors  when  kept  at  constant 
temperature  were  discharging  continuously  at  a 
steady  rate  which  varied  with  the  prevailing  tempera- 
ture. Cooling  caused  an  immediate  increase  in  the 
frequency  while  warming  led   to  the  reverse  eff"ect. 


Recent  investigations  by  Hensel  (49,  50)  on  Scyllium 
have  confirmed  Sand's  original  discovery  in  all 
details.  At  constant  temperature  the  steady  discharge 
in  single  fibers  reaches  a  maximum  of  about  65  im- 
pulses per  sec.  at  about  20°C.  The  temperature  limits 
for  steady  discharge  were  2  to  34°C.  In  this  range  of 
temperature,  sudden  cooling  produces  in  single  fibers 
a  rapid  increase  in  frequency  up  to  180  impulses  per 
sec.  followed  by  rapid  adaptation  to  a  low  steady  rate 
of  discharge.  The  ampullae  react  definitely  to  a 
change  in  temperature  of  o.o5°C.  Warming  produces 
an  immediate  decrease  or  abolition  of  the  discharge 
which  then  slowly  attains  a  new  steady  value.  The 
ampullae  are  not  sensitive  to  mechanical  stimulation 
and  they  thus  behave  qualitatively  in  every  respect 
like  the  cold  receptors  of  mammals.  Quantitatively 
they  appear  to  be  even  more  sensitive. 

The  remarkable  infrared  receptors  of  the  facial  pits 
of  the  pit  viper  (Crotalidae)  have  been  extensively 
studied  recently  by  Bullock  &  Diecke  (13).  The  nerve 
fibers  from  the  facial  pit  usually  show  a  continuous 
nonrhythmic  discharge  in  the  absence  of  environ- 
mental change.  The  adequate  stimulus  for  increa.sing 
this  activity  is  a  relative  increase  in  the  influx  or  a 
decrease  in  the  efllux  of  radiant  energy  in  the  middle 
and  long  infrared  bands.  Relative  increases  in  efflux 
or  decreases  in  influx  reduce  or  inhibit  the  steady 
discharge.  No  response  is  obtained  to  sound  vibration, 
a    number   of  chemicals   or   heat-filtered    light,    but 


JiriUM 


»IHhl|ll    l>   IW>B  ,        II       (If 


Mtl"*'*'!!   t   'I 


•   ■ — *   * 


C      .J    t    it  *  .  .     I'l    I' 


■  iii»|>tiii>UtiM>>itiiiHui 


oi  imiminiiiMNiiiiiiimiiiiM i\\\\\ 


•  ^      t.4      I,      >     H    \t>  >  »4->^-»^»VWV*-»' 


FIG.  1 1.  Afferent  spike  potentials  from  different  sensory  fibers  of  a  fine  strand  of  the  cat  lingual 
nerve  obtained  by  applying  different  stimuli  to  the  tongue.  A.  The  effect  of  a  drop  of  water  at  I4°C 
falling  on  the  tongue.  B.  First,  the  effect  of  a  faint  puff  of  air,  which  does  not  cause  any  visible  de- 
formation of  the  surface,  followed  by  the  effect  of  a  stronger  puff  of  air  which  makes  a  definite  defer 
mation.  C.  A  drop  at  8o°C  falling  upon  the  tongue.  D.  The  effect  of  pressing  a  pointed  rod  into  the 
tongue.  E.  Squirting  hot  water  (6o°C)  over  the  tongue.  [From  Zotterman  (96).] 


446 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


change  in  temperature  by  conduction  from  ambient 
media  and  mechanical  deformation  of  the  sensory 
membrane,  do  stimulate.  It  is  concluded  that  these 
are  minor  or  incidental  in  some  fibers.  Direct  measure- 
ment of  the  change  in  temperature  of  water  flowing 
over  the  membrane  necessary  to  elicit  a  response  gave 
values  of  0.003  to  o.oo5°C. 

Thus  the  nerves  of  the  facial  pit  organs  of  rattle- 
snakes are  composed  of  an  essentially  homogeneous 
population  of  warm  fibers  behaving  principally  as  do 
mammalian  warm  fibers.  But  for  the  receptors  of  the 
pit  organ  the  normal  stimulus  is  chiefly  radiant  and 
not  conducted  heat,  and  several  anatomical  properties 
adapt  it  to  a  high  sensitivity  in  terms  of  caloric  flux. 

Dodt  (19)  describes  discharges  from  the  glosso- 
pharyngeal nerve  of  the  frog  in  response  to  tempera- 
ture changes  in  the  tongue  of  more  than  3°C  even  in  a 
temperature  range  below  I5°C.  This  response  appears 
only  to  warming,  never  to  cooling.  The  response  of 
these  fibers  to  heating  the  tongue  resembles  in  many 
ways  that  of  mammalian  warm  fibers  as  well  as  that 
of  the  pit  organ  of  the  rattlesnake.  Further  experi- 
mental analysis  is,  however,  necessary  to  decide 
whether  this  response  to  heating  the  tongue  of  the 
frog  is  due  to  the  stimulation  of  nociceptive  fibers 
or  of  more  or  less  specific  warm  fibers. 

Qiiantitative  Relations  Belwem  Temperature 
Movements  and  Nerve  Fiber  Discharge 

By  use  of  well  defined  and  thermoelectrically  con- 
trolled thermal  stimuli  applied  to  the  tongue  of  the 
cat,  it  has  been  possible  to  work  out  the  fundamental 
relationships  between  the  temperature  and  the  activ- 
itv  of  the  thermal  fibers.  This  work  has  principally 
been  carried  out  in  the  writer's  laboratory  in  a  series 
of  investigations  by  Hen.sel,  Dodt  and  co-workers. 

METHODS.  For  quantitative  studies  of  cold  receptors 
we  have  used  fine  strands  of  the  cat's  lingual  nerve 
containing  only  one  or  a  few  cold  fibers.  Preparations 
containing  single  or  a  few  warm  fibers  are  best  ob- 
tained from  the  chorda  tympani  of  the  cat.  [For  the 
operative  technique  see  Zotterman  (96),  Hensel  & 
Zotterman  (53)  and  Dodt  &  Zotterman  (22).]  For 
thermal  stimulation  we  used  a  metal  thermode,  open 
at  the  top,  which  had  a  free  outflow  on  one  side  (fig. 
12).  The  thermode  had  a  gold-plated  silver  i)ottom  of 
20  X  30  mm  and  a  thickness  of  o.  i  mm.  From  above, 
two  constantly  flowing  jets  of  water  at  diff^erent 
temperatures  were  directed  on  to  the  bottom  of  the 
thermode  in  such  a  way  that  the  jets  could  suddenly 


FIG.  12.  .\ppaiatus  for  applying  rapid  temperature  changes 
to  the  surface  of  the  tongue.  Th,  thermode;  li,  silver  bottom; 
n'l  and  Ti'2,  water  jets  of  different  temperatures;  0\  and  O2, 
outflows;  S,  switch;  arrow,  movement  of  switch;  A,  axis  of 
switch;  Ti  and  To,  thermocouple  wires;  J,  junction  in  bottom 
of  thermode;  /.,  lead  strip.  [From  Hensel  et  al.  (51)-] 


be  interrupted.  In  this  way  it  was  possible  to  produce 
very  rapid  and  exact  temperature  changes  of  the 
gold-plated  silver  foil.  Soldered  on  the  thermode 
bottom  was  a  thermocouple  with  a  diameter  of  0.05 
mm  which  enabled  us  to  record  the  true  temperature 
changes  of  the  silver  foil.  Because  of  the  rapid  temper- 
ature change,  which  could  exceed  300°C  per  sec, 
the  temperature  was  recorded  either  by  a  micro- 
galvanometer  of  Moll  or  by  the  second  beam  of  the 
double  beam  cathode-ray  oscillograph  which  was 
used  for  recording  the  action  potentials.  The  thermode 
was  adjusted  on  the  tip  of  the  tongue  which  rested  on 
a  cork  plate.  It  can  easily  be  shown  both  mathemati- 
cally and  experimentally  that  a  constant  temperature 
is  reached  in  the  receptor  layer  of  the  skin  only 
negligibly  later  than  at  the  surface  of  the  tongue.  In 
many  experiments  thermocouples  were  inserted  to 
different  depths  into  the  tongue  in  order  to  record  the 
temperature  within  the  mucous  membrane. 

DISCHARGE     .\T     CONST.ANT     TEMPERATURE.     Cold   fibers. 

When  the  thermode  is  adjusted  at  a  constant  tempera- 
ture the  frequency  of  the  cold  spikes  attains  a  constant 
final  value  after  a  short  interval.  A  record  from  a 
nerve  preparation  containing  two  cold  fibers  (one 
giving  diphasic,  the  other  monophasic  spikes)  will  be 
seen  in  figure  13.  The  thermode  was  previously  kept 
for  a  long  time  at  a  constant  temperature  of  34°C. 
Even  at  this  temperature  there  was  present  a  steady 
discharge  of  the  monophasic  fiber  at  a  rate  of  9  im- 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS  447 


3» 


•c  B 


IL  I    I    I   I    I 


i*r'c 


Ur'C 


»r'C 


Fig.  13 


\n  I  iiiiniiiiii|i|iiii  mm  I 


i   I    I    i    i   i    I    I    M 


Kl    I    I   M   i    I   I    I    I    I    I  M   I    I 


I    I   I 


■itiiiiiiiiiiiiii 


I    I   I 


U5' 


Fig.  14 


itiiiutiliMiiMiKilMiilitiiHinihiiilliiiliiiitiliilllhiiMilltnini 


iitKMnnHiiiiiiiiiiiiiMilliuiiiiti 


W.5' 


IMIIiliiiliiiiiiMiiiiiMiiiil(liiililiiiiM(iiiiuiUi(ttiiiiihiiiiitniiiiniiiiiniiiMitMiniMiiiiiiMiiiiiiiiMii 


*2r'c     397' 


37 

■  IMIIII 


I  tl  II 1 1 II II  III  II  nil  II II I II M)  Mil  nil  It  IMMI II I 


lllllllllllllllll 


*2 


r'c      38.0' 


ItlhlHniUltMIMIMIIUIIIIIIIMNtlHIMIIIIIIlMIMlMMlillllllllMll IIIM  tlllnlllMMMtt  lliilllilllllllllll 


33.2' 


I    1    I    1    I    I     I     I 


null  Mil  MM  It  II  Mil  nil  II  Mil  til  111  Mil  II I 


ifr'C       25.5« 


I    I    I    I    I    M   I    i    I  lU   I   I   I   I   I 


JL 


,i    1  ,  1,1 1  I    II    I    I    i 


FIG.  13.  Spike  potentials  from  two  cold  fibers  recorded  on  cooling  the  tongue  of  the  cat  from 
34°  to  32 °C.  .4,  the  temperature  drop;  B,  after  i  min. ,  C,  after  2  min.;  D,  after  4  min.;  E,  after  15 
min.  [From  Hensel  &  Zottcrman  (54).] 

FIG.  14.  .Spikes  recorded  from  a  single  cold  fiber  (cat)  at  different  constant  temperatures. 
Time  marks,   50  cps.   [From  Hensel  &  Zotterman  (54).] 


pulses  per  sec.  from  which  the  three  spikes  can  be 
seen  before  the  coohng  starts  (fig.  1 3).  The  thermode 
was  then  quickly  cooled  down  to  32°C.  Immediately 
the  frequency  of  the  monophasic  fiber  rose  to  35 
impulses  per  sec.  and  simultaneously  there  was  a 
discharge  of  thesecond  diphasic  fiber  which,  however, 
after  a  few  seconds  ceased  again,  while  the  discharge 
of  the  first  fiber  adjusted  itself  to  a  final  constant  fre- 
quency which  after  i  min.  attained  a  value  of  9.3 
impulses  per  sec.  After  that  there  is  practically  no 
more  change.  The  cold  receptor  goes  on  discharging 
at  a  fairly  regular  rhythm  for  minutes  or  even  hours 
with  remarkable  constancy  if  the  temperature  of  the 
surface  of  the  tongue  is  kept  constant. 

Figure  14  gives  an  example  of  the  discharge  of  a 
single  cold  fiber  after  adjustment  to  a  constant  final 
value  of  frequency  at  different  constant  temperatures. 
At  a  temperature  of  4i.3°C  there  is  no  discharge  in 
this  fiber  while  already  at  a  constant  temperature  of 
40.5°C  there  is  a  low  frequency  of  about  i   impulse 


per  sec.  The  upper  limit  at  which  a  steady  discharge 
of  the  cold  fiber  appears,  called  the  steady  threshold 
temperature  for  this  particular  cold  fiber,  lay  between 
41.3°  and  40.5°C,  i.e.  above  the  ordinary  blood 
temperature.  At  this  temperature  (38°C)  the  fre- 
quency of  the  steady  discharge  was  5  impulses  per  sec. 
and  the  maximum  about  3o°C.  Below  this  tempera- 
ture the  steady  discharge  declines  and  at  lower  tem- 
peratures the  discharge  generally  becomes  irregular, 
occurring  in  beats  of  two  or  three  impulses.  Between 
15°  and  io°C  the  average  discharge  increases  again 
(17)  to  disappear  entirely  between  12°  and  io°C. 
No  steady  discharge  of  any  cold  fibers  has  been  seen 
at  a  temperature  below  8°C. 

The  diagram  in  figure  1 5  shows  the  steady  dis- 
charge of  a  cold  fiber  as  a  function  of  the  temperature. 
The  experiiTient  was  conducted  in  such  a  way  that 
the  frequency  was  determined  at  definite  teinperature 
steps  from  warm  to  cold.  After  reaching  the  lowest 
temperature — in  about  2  hr. — the  impulse  frequency 


448 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


10  r-  Jmp/iec 


5  - 


from  40  -  20    C 
..       20  -  40    C 


25 


10 


i5 


•C     ♦<? 


FIG.  15.  Impulse  frequency  of  the  steady  discharge  of  a  single 
cold  fiber  as  a  function  of  the  temperature  of  the  tongue  surface. 
The  frequencies  were  first  measured  at  temperature  steps  from 
40°  to  '20°C  and  then  again  in  reverse  order.  [From  Hensel  & 
Zotterman  (54).] 


was  recorded  once  again  when  the  temperature  was 
raised  in  the  same  steps  as  before.  It  is  seen  that  the 
points  lie  on  the  same  curve.  Even  after  hours  of 
experiments  exposing  the  receptors  to  widely  difTering 
temperatures,  the  steady  di.scharge  appears  with  the 
same  frequency  when  the  temperature  of  the  tongue 
is  restored  to  the  initial  value.  The  impulse  frequency 
of  the  steady  discharge  of  the  cold  receptors  thus 
depends  entirely  on  the  temperature. 

The  steady  discharge  of  cold  fibers  shows  a  maxi- 
mum frequency  of  about  10  impulses  per  sec.  The 
site  of  this  inaximum  on  the  temperature  scale  varies 
in  different  fibers  between  20°  and  34°C,  while  the 
upper  and  lower  temperature  liinits  in  extreme  cases 
reach  10°  and  41  °C.  The  total  frequency  of  cold 
impulses  in  the  nerve,  which  is  the  sum  of  the  dis- 
charges from  the  single  cold  fibers,  reaches  its  inaxi- 
mum (fig.  16)  at  a  temperature  between  15  and  20  °C 

(54)- 

Warm  fibers.  Judging  from  the  relative  spike  height 

the  fibers  inediating  warmth  were  conceived  to  be 
somewhat  larger  in  diameter  than  the  cold  fibers  (96). 
Preparations  containing  only  warm  fibers  display  a 
steady  discharge  to  constant  temperatures  between 
20°  and  47°C.  In  single  filler  preparations  the  fre- 
quency of  this  steady  discharge  varies  in  a  consistent 
manner  with  the  temperature,  although  the  maxi- 
mum discharge  as  well  as  the  upper  and  lower  tem- 
perature limits  vary  somewhat  (fig.  17).  The  max- 
imum usually  was  found  between  37.5°  and  40°C. 
At  higher  temperatures  the  steady  discharge  falls  ofT 


rather  steeply.  Above  47°C  and  below  20°C  no  steady 
discharge  was  noticed.  The  maximum  frequency 
varied  in  single  warm  fiber  units  between  1.5  and  3.7 
impulses  per  sec.  (23).  The  discharge  was  never  as 
regular  as  that  of  the  cold  fibers  which  may  depend  on 
the  comparatively  low  frequency  and  also  on  the 
possibility  that  the  warm  fibers  may  divide  peripher- 
ally to  supply  more  than  one  end  organ. 

The  low  sensitivity  of  warm  receptors  to  tempera- 
tures between  20°  and  30°C  aho  has  an  important 
bearing  on  the  interpretation  of  Weber's  phenomenon 
of  persisting  cold  sensation  (cf.  page  443).  When  the 
cold  object  is  removed  from  the  skin  there  is  a  distinct 
pause  in  the  cold  sensation  due  to  the  postexcitatory 
depression  of  the  cold  receptors.  When  the  cold  sensa- 
tion then  slowly  reappears,  although  the  temperature 
of  the  skin  is  gradually  rising,  there  will  be  very  little 
interference  from  the  scattered  warm  receptors.  Thus 
the  steady  discharge  of  impulses  from  the  cold  recep- 
tors which  display  their  maximum  sensitivity  in  just 
this  temperature  range  25°  to  30°C  will  stand  out  still 
more  conspicuously. 


Jmplc,ec 
100  r- 


— o—  Single  fiber 
— •-  4-5  fibers 
Itl  10-20  fibers 


55     'C   ^ 


FIG.  16.  Total  impulse  frequency  of  the  steady  discharge  in 
different  preparations  of  the  cat  lingua!  nerve  as  a  function  of 
the  temperature  of  the  tongue  surface.  [From  Hensel  &  Zotter- 
man (54).] 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


449 


^'"Plsec 


a 

7 

— 

6 

— 

5 

— 

4 

— 

3 

— 

2 

— 

1 



— o—  Single  fibers 
_o—  Two  fibers 


50  °C 


Fig.    17.   Graph  showing  frequency  of  the  steady  discharge  of  different  single  and  dual  warm 
fiber  preparations  as  a  function  of  the  temperature  of  the  tongue  surface.  [From  Dodt  &  Zotterman 

(23)-] 


RESPONSE    OF    THERMAL    RECEPTORS    TO    TEMPERATURE 

CHANGES.  For  the  investigation  of  the  influence  of 
temporal  changes  in  temperature  on  the  impulse  fre- 
quency, rapid  changes  from  one  constant  temperature 
level  to  another  constant  level  were  used.  A  purely 
rectangular  shape  of  the  temperature  rise  curve  could 
not  be  obtained  as  the  equalization  of  the  receptor 
temperature  takes  a  certain  time,  but  a  very  rapid 
adjustment  to  a  constant  value  was  ensured. 

Cold  fibers.  Sudden  cooling  of  the  tongue  produces, 
as  is  seen  in  figure  18,  a  rapid  discharge  of  the  cold 
fibers  which  quickly  declines  to  the  final  value  of  the 
steady  discharge  characteristic  of  the  prevalent  tem- 
perature. The  maximum  response  of  a  .single  cold 
fiber  seen  when  applying  a  very  rapid  cooling  from 
40°  to  2°C  was  140  impulses  per  .sec.  which  is  about 
15  times  as  high  as  the  maximum  frequency  recorded 
at  a  constant  temperature. 

In  contrast  to  the  steady  discharge,  the  frequency 
of  which  is  determined  solely  by  the  temperature,  the 
maximum  frequency  at  temperature  changes  is  not 
so  much  dependent  on  the  initial  or  final  temperature 
as  on  the  rate  of  the  temperature  change  (dd/di). 
Rapid  cooling  can  thus  produce  a  discharge  from  cold 
receptors  even  in  a  rather  warm  temperature  region 
above  the  upper  temperature  limit  of  the  steady  dis- 
charge as  is  shown  in  figure  18. 

The  maximum  rate  of  discharge  of  the  cold  fibers 
in  response  to  rapid  cooling  is,  however,  not  exclu- 
sively determined  by  the  rate  of  cooling  (dd/di),  as 


is  evident  from  figure  igi?.  Applying  equal  tempera- 
ture drops  of  2°C  at  various  initial  temperatures,  it 
was  found  that  identical  intracutaneous  temperature 
changes  elicited  different  grades  of  excitation  in  the 
cold  receptors  depending  upon  the  range  of  tempera- 
ture within  which  the  change  occurred. 

Rapid  warming  of  the  cold  receptors  to  a  constant 
temperature  leads  to  an  immediate  cessation  of  the 
steady  discharge.  If  this  temperature  lies  below  the 
upper  temperature  limit  of  the  steady  discharge,  the 
impulses  reappear  and  adjust  themselves  at  a  fre- 
quency corresponding  to  the  prevailing  temperature 
(fig.  19).  The  length  of  this  pause  caused  by  warming 
the  cold  receptors  depends  upon  the  rate  of  warming 
and  the  range  of  temperature.  Thus,  while  rapid 
cooling  leads  to  an  'overshooting'  excitation  of  the 
cold  receptors,  rapid  warming  of  these  receptors 
produces  an  'overshooting'  inhibition. 

If  the  warming  is  small  or  follows  at  a  relatively 
low  rate,  the  cold  impulses  may  not  disappear  at  all 
but  occur  only  at  another  frequency.  Thus  cold  im- 
pulses were  shown  to  appear  even  during  warming  of 
the  cold  receptors.  This  offers  a  ready  explanation  of 
Weber's  'persisting  cold'  sensation  (cf  page  443) 
that  below  a  certain  temperature  cold  sensations  may 
occur  even  when  the  temperature  of  the  receptor 
layer  of  the  skin  is  rising.  The  objection  ba.sed  on  the 
spread  of  the  cooling  to  surrounding  parts  of  the  skin 
was  quite  pointless  in  these  experiments  since,  when 
thin  nerve  preparations  are  used,  the  receptive  field 


450 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


sec    50 


FIG.  i8.  Impulse  frequency  of  a  single  cold  fiber  in  the  cat 
lingual  nerve  during  induction  of  rapid  temperature  changes 
in  the  tongue.  A,  From  32°  to  30°C;  B,  from  40°  to  38°C;  C, 
from  42°  to  40°C;  D,  from  44°  to  42 °C.  Cooling  starts  at  time 
zero.  [From  Hensel  &  Zotterman  (54).] 


of  the  cold  fibers  is  far  smaller  than  the  surface  of  the 
thermode  in  contact  with  the  tongue.  The  cold  recep- 
tors involved  can  thus  be  influenced  only  by  the 
temperature  changes  below  the  thermode  and  not  at 
all  bv  any  spread  of  cooling  outside  the  thermode. 

]\'arm  fibers.  The  discharge  of  the  warm  fibers  in 
response  to  thermal  stimuli  displays  essentially  the 
same  characteristic  features  as  does  that  of  the  cold 
fibers  but  in  the  reverse  order.  Thus  rapid  warming 
to  a  constant  level  produces  an  overshooting  discharge 
after  which  the  discharge  adjusts  itself  fairly  quickly 
into  an  irregular  steady  rhythm.  The  initial  volley 
(fig.  20)  appears  after  latencies  varying  from  0.15  to 
0.55  sec.  which  was  taken  as  indicating  that  the 
warm  receptors  were  situated  at  varying  depths  in  the 
tongue.  Short  latencies  were  observed  particularly 
for  temperature  rises  from  ;57°  to  40°C,  whereas 
latencies  for  corresponding  rises  in  the  range  of  25° 
to  35°C  were  considerably  longer.  The  discharge  of  a 
single  warm  receptor  displays  a  much  higher  initial 
frequency  compared  with  that  of  a  cold  receptor 
exposed  to  a  corresponding  drop  in  temperature,  and 
the  sequence  of  impulses  thereafter  is  interrupted  by 
sudden  pauses  in  the  discharge.  The  mean  value  of 


the  frequency  falls,  however,  in  an  exponential  way 
as  does  the  discharge  of  the  cold  receptors. 

Figure  21  shows  simultaneous  records  from  a 
strand  of  the  chorda  tympani  (above)  and  from  a 
strand  of  the  lingual  ner\-e  (below).  The  former 
preparation  contained  warm  fibers  only  while  the 
lingual  strand  possessed  cold  fibers  and  one  touch 
fiber.  At  a  constant  temperature  of  23 °C  there  is  a 
steady  discharge  of  cold  fibers  in  the  lingual  prepara- 
tion (below)  while  hardly  any  spikes  are  recorded 
from  the  chorda  strand.  When  the  temperature  was 
raised  to  38.5°C,  a  discharge  of  the  warm  fibers 
occurred  while  the  steady  discharge  of  the  cold  fibers 
disappeared.  A  quick  return  of  the  temperature  to 
23°C  produced  a  small  oflf-efTect  from  the  warm 
fibers  at  the  same  time  as  the  cold  fibers  started  their 
firing.  When  this  procedure  was  repeated  after  6  sec. 
(fig.  21),  hardly  any  change  in  the  response  could  be 
noticed.  If  however  the  temperature  was  raised  to 
44.8°C,  a  certain  difiference  in  the  warm  fiber  response 
was  noticed.  First  of  all,  the  sudden  rise  elicits  a  much 
stronger  initial  frequency  of  warm  impulses.  Secondly, 
when    the    temperature   dropped    there    appeared    a 


Time  t 


FIG.  19.  Graphs  showing  .-J,  impulse  frequency  of  the  steady 
discharge  of  a  single  cold  fiber ;  B,  frequency  of  a  sudden  tem- 
perature drop  at  different  temperatures.  [From  Hensel  (48).] 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


451 


A   40     °^ 


'  .I»>1*.HW|M||B  I      \ 


i*-^ 


¥m    w^ 


iuiuij.g£'..jniiiiu»iiaiiiiuiiiiiuiiiu»i»»HitiMUiiiuiituiJi»'M"MiillllllllttUllMlHii  l««iii'i'i»"i'''«iui!ilWIII«wwwm 


B 


45\   OC 


^ 


•^ 


^U^ 


^4^ 


^ 


iM^ 


iijiUtaiiyijtiuu)itiiuiiiuiiiiiMHii)iiiiuujiuuiuuuiuuiiuyiu)u^^^^     ifiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiifiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiMtwmim 


'^ 


^m^ 


■!■ 


iiijiiu  J5  Liidiiiii'':ii)uiiuuiiiJuy[MUUUiiM  muuiuuuiuuyiuuiuiHiiuuuuuuiiu 


FIG.  20.  Records  showing  the  warm  fiber  response  to  sudden  heating  and  cooling  of  the  cat  tongue 
at  different  levels.  .4,  temperature  rise,  14.2°  to  38.6°C,  latency  0.55  sec.  B,  25.3°  to  38.6°C,  latency 
0.46  sec.  C,  30°  to  38.6°C,  latency  0.42  sec.  D,  34.4°  to  38.6°C,  latency  0.47  sec.  The  latency  of  the 
off-effect,  'paradoxical  warmth,'  varied  between  o.io  and  0.15  sec.  [From  Dodt  &  Zotterman  (23).] 


A    ^"^. 


hMPViMMMMi 


H     MM>»4«|ii( 


'I    >l    I    I 


Q  40     ^ 


NupT    I    li  I   Hill  ^  »(i»liih  III  iK»|fi  I    I      ij 


"2** 


C    45     OC 


I  i    ii0»if<mniii^ I  III  iiw(i'»»M 


MM 


•gi*?^ 


iiiiiiiii!iMiiiiiiiriiiiiiiiiiiiriiii!iMiMihMMiMiiiiiiiiiii'iiiiiiiiiiitiiiiiMtiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiMiiiiiiiiniiiiniiniiiiniiiniiiuiiiiiii 


FIG.  21.  Simultaneous  recording  from  a  warm  fiber  preparation  of  the  chorda  tympani  and  a 
cold  fiber  preparation  of  the  lingual  nerve.  A  and  B,  a  rise  from  23°  to  38.5°C  and  back;  C  and  D, 
from  23.8°  to  44.8°C  and  back.  B  and  D  are  recorded  6  sec.  after  the  previous  record.  [From  Dodt 
&  Zotterman  (23).] 


Strong  off-discharge  from  the  warm  fibers.  As  can  be 
seen  in  figure  21,  this  effect  occurred  after  a  very  short 
latency,  much  shorter  even  than  that  of  the  cold  fiber 
discharge.  When  the  heating  was  repeated  within  6 
sec,  the  initial  warm  fiber  response  to  heating  to 
44.8°C  each  time  was  very  much  reduced  while  the 


off-effect    from    the    warm    fibers    remained    almost 
unchanged. 

This  off-effect  of  the  warm  fibers  was  observed 
whenever  the  temperature  dropped  about  8°  to 
I5°C.  It  is  more  conspicuous  the  more  rapid  the 
change    in    temperature,    but    it    appeared    even    at 


452 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH'iSIOLGGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


rather  slow  rates  of  temperature  change.  The  response 
soon  fades  away  when  the  temperature  is  kept  below 
20°C  and  is  thus  always  of  a  phasic  character  in  con- 
trast to  the  steady  paradoxical  discharge  of  cold  fibers 
when  exposed  to  constant  temperatures  above  45°C. 
The  fact  that  the  latency  of  the  warm  fiber  response  to 
sudden  cooling  is  only  about  a  third  of  the  shortest 
latency  of  its  discharge  to  warming  induced  Dodt  & 
Zotterman  (23)  to  consider  whether  the  former  re- 
sponse of  the  warm  fibers  is  due  to  an  excitation  of  the 
warm  receptor  or  to  its  nerve  fiber.  As  was  originally 
shown  by  Bernhard  &  Granit  (7)  rapid  cooling  of  a 
nerve  trunk  excites  class  A  fibers  directly  and  such  a 
rapid  cooling  also  stimulates  the  endings  of  mechano- 
ceptive  fibers  (53).  This  nerve  fiber  discharge  in  every 
respect  is  of  the  same  character  as  the  discharge  of 
warm  fibers  to  cooling.  It  has  a  relatively  short 
latency,  the  length  of  which  varies  with  the  rate  of 
cooling;  the  discharge  is  phasic,  i.e.  there  is  no  steady 
discharge.  When,  however,  the  thermode  was  placed 
on  the  other  side  of  the  tongue  where  the  more  central 
part  of  the  lingual  nerve  runs  closely  under  the  sur- 
face, no  discharge  of  the  warm  fibers  could  be  seen 
when  this  surface  was  suddenly  cooled  from  45°  to 
25°C. 

An  old  question  in  the  field  of  sensory  physiology 
is  whether  sensation  is  in  part  due  to  direct  stimula- 
tion of  the  .sensory  nerve  fibers  as  well  as  of  the  recep- 
tors. In  the  function  of  thermoreception  this  question 
is,  as  we  have  seen,  of  particular  importance.  For  that 
reason  Dodt  (18)  made  a  thorough  study  of  thermo- 
sensitivity  of  A  fibers  in  the  lingual  nerve  and  com- 
pared the  responses  in  specific  cold,  warm  and 
mechanoceptive  fibers  upon  thermal  stimulation  of 
the  surface  of  tongue  and  of  the  lingual  nerve,  re- 
spectively. He  found  that  all  three  types  of  afferent 
fibers  were  phasically  excited  by  local  cooling  of  the 
nerve  trunk.  In  mechanoceptive  fibers  this  occurred 
whenever  the  temperature  drop  was  of  a  sufficient 
magnitude,  regardless  of  the  final  value,  the  effect 
being  optimally  elicited  when  the  nerve  was  at  an 
initial  temperature  between  35°  to  40 °C.  Cold  fibers, 
however,  were  excited  only  when  the  nerve  trunk  was 
cooled  to  below  a  certain  threshold  value  of  about 
20°C.  Warm  fibers  were  mixed  in  their  reactions, 
some  responding  like  mechanoceptive  fibers,  others 
having  a  distinct  threshold.  Warming  of  the  nerve 
trunk  never  led  to  excitation  of  sensory  A  fibers. 

The  cold  and  the  mechanoceptive  fibers  could  be 
blocked  by  low  and  high  temperatures,  the  cold  fibers 
being  blocked  below  16°  to  25°C  and  above  50°  to 


52 °C,  whereas  the  thresholds  in  mechanoceptors  were 
in  both  types  of  block  5°  to  8°C  lower. 

Cold  fibers,  excited  by  cooling  the  nerve  to  tem- 
peratures insufficient  to  cause  blocking,  show  follow- 
ing the  phasic  excitation  an  impulse-free  interval,  the 
duration  of  which  varies  directly  with  the  length  of 
the  excitatory  burst  and  inversely  with  the  back- 
ground frequency  of  impulses  coming  from  the  recep- 
tor. 

These  findings  suggest  that  under  physiological 
conditions  normal  and  paradoxical  sensations  of  cold 
are  due  to  the  stimulation  of  the  thermal  receptors  or 
the  nerve  fibers  included  in  the  end  organ  and  never 
to  a  direct  stimulation  of  their  myelinated  nerve 
fibers. 

PAR.^DOXic.'^L  DISCHARGES.  When  the  temperature  of 
the  tongue  is  raised  above  45  °C,  a  steady  discharge  of 
cold  fibers  is  produced  (24).  This  impulse  activity 
increases  slowly  and  attains  a  level  corresponding  to 
the  prevailing  temperature.  This  paradoxical  dis- 
charge begins  at  45  °C  and  a  maximum  frequency  of 
7  to  7.5  impulses  per  sec.  is  attained  at  5o°C.  The 
lower  threshold  temperature  of  this  paradoxical  dis- 
charge lies  about  5°C  above  the  upper  limit  of  the 
usual  range  of  temperature  within  which  the  cold 
receptors  display  a  steady  discharge  (fig.  22).  Para- 
doxical excitation  of  cold  receptors  at  temperatures 
below  45°C  does  not  occur.  Thus  the  cold  sensation 
which  appears  after  a  rise  in  the  skin  temperature 
from  20°  to  35°C  as  described  by  Thunberg  (88)  is 
not  due  to  any  paradoxical  excitation  but  to  a  reap- 
pearance of  the  usual  steady  discharge  of  cold  fibers 
when  the  temperature  approaches  the  final  value  of 


FIG.  22.  Graphs  showing  impulse  frequency  of  the  steady 
discharge  of  a  single  cold  fiber  iopen  circles)  and  of  a  single  warm 
fiber  QfiUed  circles')  as  a  function  of  the  temperature  of  the  recep- 
tors within  the  range  of  10  to  50°C.  [From  Zotterman  (99).] 


THERMAL    SENSATIONS 


453 


35°C.  This  phenomenon  is  only  another  example  of 
'persisting  cold  sensation'  (cf.  page  443).  The  low 
sensitivity  of  the  warm  receptors  and  the  relatively 
high  sensitivity  of  the  cold  receptors  between  20°  and 
30°C  has  an  important  bearing  on  our  interpretation 
of  the  Weber  phenomenon  of  persisting  cold  sensation. 
When  a  cold  object  is  removed  from  the  skin,  there  is 
an  obvious  pause  in  the  cold  sensation  due  to  the 
postexcitatory  depression  of  the  cold  receptors.  When 
the  cold  sensation  then  slowly  reappears,  although  the 
temperature  of  the  skin  is  gradually  rising,  there  will 
be  very  little  interference  from  the  rather  scattered 
warm  receptors.  Thus  the  steady  discharge  of  im- 
pulses from  the  cold  receptors  which  display  their 
ma,\imum  sensitivity  in  just  this  temperature  region, 
25°  to  30°C,  will  stand  out  still  more  conspicuously. 

The  question  concerning  the  real  existence  of  a 
paradoxical  warmth  sensation  brought  about  by  cool- 
ing has  been  the  subject  of  much  discussion  C72).  The 
reason  for  this  is  now  quite  obvious.  We  have  to  con- 
sider not  only  that  cooling  of  the  skin  stimulates 
numerous  cold  receptors  and  that  the  ensuing  cold 
sensation  thus  will  mask  the  paradoxical  warmth 
sensation  ijut  also  the  fact  that  the  'paradoxical' 
response  of  the  warm  fibers  is  of  phasic  character  and 
soon  fades  away. 

Thus  we  can  conclude  that  the  paradoxical  sensa- 
tion of  cold  experienced  when  the  skin  is  heated  to  a 
temperature  between  45°  and  50°C  has  its  ph)'siologi- 
cal  analogy  in  a  steady  discharge  of  specific  cold 
fibers. 

The  paradoxical  warmth  sensation  which  generalh 
is  masked  by  an  intense  cold  sensation  has  its  counter- 
part in  a  phasic  discharge  of  specific  warm  fibers  to 
the  cooling. 

Effect  of  temperatures  above  47°c.  The  fact  that 
the  steady  discharge  of  the  warm  receptors  generalh 
disappears  at  a  temperature  above  47°C  must  mean 
that  above  this  temperature  the  quality  of  sensation 
which  generally  is  described  as  hot  has  little  to  do 
with  the  feeling  of  warmth  (cf.  page  444).  Alrutz' 
(2)  suggestion  that  the  sensation  of  heat  was  a  mixed 
sensation  of  warmth  and  paradoxical  cold  must  be 
revised  to  some  degree.  When  the  skin  is  suddenly 
heated  from  35°  to  50°C,  there  occurs  first  a  sudden 
transient  discharge  of  warm  fibers  accompanied  by  a 
paradoxical  cold  fiber  discharge  which  continues  as 
long  as  the  temperature  is  kept  at  this  level.  To  this 
paradoxical  discharge  of  cold  fibers  a  discharge  of 
pain  fibers  is  gradually  added  (96,  97).  Skouby  (So) 


has  recently  found  that  the  subjective  pain  threshold 
lies  at  temperatures  of  47.  i  °  to  48.5°C.  Thus  it  can  be 
concluded  that,  when  temperatures  of  above  47 °C  are 
applied  and  after  the  temperature  change  in  the  skin 
has  ceased,  the  sensation  of  heat  then  experienced  is 
the  resultant  of  a  mixed  inflow  of  paradoxical  cold 
and  pain  impulses.  This  sensation  is  thus  initiated  only 
by  warm  and  paradoxical  cold  impulses  and  to  the 
persisting  paradoxical  cold  discharge,  pain  impulses 
are  gradually  added  as  the  temperature  is  kept  at  a 
constant  value  above  47  °C.  At  still  higher  tempera- 
tures the  heat  will  destroy  the  fibers.  Heating  the  skin 
to  more  than  50°C  very  quickly  not  only  inactivates 
the  mechanoceptive  fibers  in  the  tongue  (96)  but 
also  causes  the  steady  paradoxical  discharge  of  the 
cold  fiber  to  disappear  leaving  the  signalling  duty 
entirely  to  pain  fibers.  This  course  of  events  was  fully 
confirmed  by  recent  experiments  of  Dodt  (19). 

intracut.anequs  gradient.  In  order  to  investigate 
the  importance  of  the  intracutaneous  temperature 
gradient  for  the  stimulation  of  the  thermoceptors 
Hensel  &  Zotterman  (55)  recorded  the  action  poten- 
tials from  the  cold  fibers  of  the  lingual  nerve  of  the  cat 
when  cold  stimuli  were  applied  to  the  tongue  so  as  to 
cause  negative  or  positive  intracutaneous  temperature 
gradients  (cf.  page  446).  The  nerve  preparations 
chosen  were  those  containing  cold  fibers  supplying 
only  the  upper  surface  of  the  tip  of  the  tongue.  In  order 
to  produce  negative  or  positive  temperature  gradients, 
the  tongue  was  cooled  from  either  the  upper  or  the 
lower  surface,  respectively,  the  temperature  on  both 
sides  of  the  tongue  being  recorded  thermoelectrically. 

The  cooling  of  the  upper  surface  immediately  gave 
rise  to  a  strong  discharge  of  cold  spikes  which  instantly 
disappeared  on  rewarming  (fig.  23).  On  cooling  of 
the  lower  side  no  impulses  appeared  at  first  ijut  within 
1.5  to  3  sec,  when  the  cold  had  penetrated  the  tongue 
and  reached  the  upper  surface,  cold  impulses  appeared 
with  increasing  frequency.  On  rewarming  of  the  lower 
surface  the  cold  impulses  persisted  at  first  until  the 
upper  surface  was  also  warmed  again.  In  some  experi- 
ments the  cooling  of  the  receptor  laser  was  produced 
by  injecting  cold  solutions  into  the  lingual  artery. 
This  way  of  cooling  produced  the  same  cold  receptor 
discharges  as  cooling  the  surface.  The  participation 
of  deep  thennoreceptors  could  be  entirely  excluded 
in  these  experiments,  and  the  arrest  of  the  blood  flow 
in  the  tongue  had  no  primary  influence  on  these 
findings. 

These  experiments  demonstrate  that  the  stimulation 


454 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


37 


29 


]   I  I  iiiiir rilif(»i)*iiM>Wiil>liii|iMiiill«»iil»iiMiilil>MNII'lll<ll 


sf 


vc  B 


29'- 


■^^— *— *     I  ii>ii  I  iiimiiiiiiiiiii  I  mill 


('Weber's  deception'Ji.  The  pressure  sensation  caused 
by  cooling  the  skin  has  been  the  subject  of  thorough 
examination  by  later  workers.  Kiesow  (62)  con- 
firmed the  existence  of  'Weber's  deception'  and  also 
succeeded  in  provoking  sensations  of  pressure  by 
application  of  ether  and  chloroform  to  the  skin,  and 
Goldscheider  &  Hahn  (33),  experimenting  with  vari- 
ous solutions  and  with  cooled  air,  came  to  the  conclu- 
sion that  the  mcchanorcccptors  could  be  stimulated 
by  cooling. 


FIG.  23.  .Simultaneous  records  of  cold  impulses  from  receptor 
field  on  upper  surface  of  cat  tongue  and  of  temperature  of  both 
surfaces.  A,  on  cooling  of  the  upper  surface;  B,  on  cooling  of 
the  lower  surface;  a,  temperature  of  the  upper  surface;  b,  tem- 
perature of  lower  surface.  Time  marks,  1 5  cps.  [From  Hensel  & 
Zotterman  (55)-] 


of  the  cold  receptors  does  not  depend  upon  the  direc- 
tion or  slope  of  intracutaneous  temperature  gradient. 
Thus  temperature  gradients  between  blood  vessels 
and  the  receptors,  which  were  suggested  as  the  ade- 
quate stimulus,  cannot  be  decisive  since  arrest  of  the 
circulation  for  a  minute  or  so  did  not  notably  change 
the  results  obtained  by  the  retrograde  temperature 
gradient. 

RESPONSE  OF  MECH.iiNORECEPTORS  TO  THERM.'^L  STIMU- 
LATION. When  the  tongue  of  the  cat  or  dog  is  cooled, 
it  is  generally  possible  only  to  record  small  cold  spikes 
in  the  lingual  nerve,  whereas  the  large  touch  and  pres- 
sure spikes  cannot  be  elicited  by  cooling.  However,  in 
a  few  cases  cooling  also  sets  up  relatively  large  spikes 
in  the  lingual  nerve.  Hensel  &  Zotterman  (53)  investi- 
gated this  phenomenon  further  and  demonstrated  that 
these  larger  spikes  derived  from  mechanoceptive 
nerve  fibers.  These  large  spikes  usually  appear  only 
with  severe  cooling  and  disappear  within  a  few  seconds 
at  a  constant  low  temperature,  whereas  small  cold 
spikes  appear  with  slight  cooling  and  persist  at  con- 
stant temperatures  for  long  periods  (fig.  24).  It  was 
shown  that  this  activity  of  mechanoceptive  fibers 
could  not  ije  due  to  secondary  mechanical  stimulation 
of  the  pressure  receptors  by  local  vasoconstriction  nor 
to  stimulation  of  the  nerve  trunk  by  cooling.  It  is  only 
medium-sized  mechanoceptive  fibers  (8  to  10  /li) 
which  were  stimulated  by  cooling  the  surface;  the 
larger  pressure  fibers  (12  to  15  m)  were  not  excited. 

These  findings  offer  a  ready  explanation  for  the 
well-known  phenomenon  first  described  by  Weber  (94) 
that    cold    weights    seem    heavier    than    warm    ones 


Injiiience  of  Nonthermal  Agencies 

That  menthol  evokes  cold  .sensations  when  applied 
on  the  tongue  as  well  as  on  the  skin  is  a  well-known 
experience  which  has  been  exploited  in  manifold  ways. 
It  has  likewise  long  been  known  that  these  cold  sensa- 
tions are  not  caused  by  physical  cooling  of  the  skin  or 
the  mucous  membranes  but  by  some  chemical  action 
directly  on  the  cold  receptors  (32).  Hensel  &  Zotter- 
man (56)  recorded  the  discharge  of  cold  fibers  after 
the  application  of  menthol  solutions  upon  the  tongue 
using  well  defined  thermal  stimulation. 


^    *0r', 


tiipiiiiiiii  iiniiiiiiiiiiiijutiiiiiiiiiyyiimy 


""""""""""" '""" ■'■■ """ ■"""■" """■ 


■tOOuV 


..MJJJ.I.L:; 


III.,    1 


tiiMliiiiiimiHifc 


C^X'C 


50 


JHlMtwaAid&iii«^4«t«ittjate 


MMlwilllikMiiLd^^ 


£*0 

50 


tmtt^u^mttmJm^iikiii^ditmdtiMithit 


FIG.  24.  Records  from  a  thin  strand  of  the  cat  lingual  nerve 
obtained  on  applying  mechanical  and  thermal  stimuli  to  the 
tongue.  The  Ihin  line  shows  the  temperature  of  the  surface  of  the 
tongue.  A,  pressure;  B,  cooling  from  41°  to  22°C;  C,  cooling 
from  41  °  to  26°C;  D,  cooling  from  41  °  to  2()°C;  E,  cooling  from 
41°  to  32  °C.  Time  marks,  50  cps.  [From  Hensel  &  Zotterman 
(53)-] 


THERMAL   SENSATIONS 


455 


'Mill iMIlllllllllllMMIIIIIIIIIIlllllllinillllllllllllllllhllllllllllllMlllllllllllllMllUlllinilllllllll 


*oi'c    Menthol  1  10.000 

tei4yiUitiiiiiiitiiiiiiiiiiliiiiiiililuijiiii>iiiiiiiii 


iiiiiiiiiiitiiiiiiiilliiniiii 


'<:  Menthol  1  10.000      ,,   ,  ,         ,    i     ,  , 

iyilMUmiiiJ.4  11  111  iUiJ  "    lioUL 


iiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiMiiiiiiiiiiiiMiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiniiiiiiiiiiiiiiiuiinihiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiHiiiiiiiiiihiiiiiiiii 


•wr-f   Menthol  1  10.000 

f"-    1  1  -[-M  1    '    ■■^'^^^"'^ i.ii.iii>iiiy.y.ti 


lillllllillinliMilinliii 


FIG.  25.  Action  potentials  from  cold  and  warm  fibers  in  a 
thin  strand  of  the  cat  lingual  nerve  after  the  application  of 
menthol  solution  to  the  tongue.  Under  the  action  of  menthol 
(i :  10,000)  there  is  at  40°C  strong  discharge  of  cold  libers  which 
disappears  on  warming,  to  be  followed  by  discharge  from  a 
warm  fiber.  [From  Hensel  &  Zottcrman  (56).] 


substances  into  the  skin  produced  an  increased  num- 
ber of  cold  spots,  Dodt  el  al.  (21)  investigated  the 
effect  on  single  thermal  fibers.  Minute  amounts  of 
acetylcholine  shift  the  temperature  range  of  the  steady 
discharge  of  cold  fibers  towards  the  warm  side  and 
increase  definitely  the  rate  of  the  steady  discharge  of 
the  receptors  inside  the  normal  range  of  temperature. 
Larger  amounts  produce  a  depression  of  the  steady 
discharge  and  a  narrowing  of  the  temperature  range 
recorded.  Corresponding  results  were  found  with 
warm  fiber  preparations. 

Dodt  (20)  has  recently  investigated  the  influence 
of  carbon  dio.xide  on  the  thermal  receptors.  An  in- 
crease of  the  pClOa  reduced  the  rate  of  the  steady  dis- 
charge of  cold  receptors,  whereas  it  caused  an  in- 
crease of  the  steady  discharge  of  warm  receptors.  The 
regulating  structures  will  thus,  under  the  action  of 
carbon  dioxide,  receive  a  false  picture  of  the  actual 
thermal  conditions  in  the  periphery  which  will  lead 
to  a  fall  of  the  rectal  temperature  without  any  sub- 
jective discomfort. 


Aqueous  menthol  solutions  of  1:10,000  lead  to 
strong  steady  discharge  of  the  tongue  cold  receptors 
at  constant  warm  temperatures  at  which  without 
menthol  there  is  no  discharge  (fig.  25).  At  lower 
temperatures  at  which  the  cold  receptors  are  steadily 
discharging  without  menthol,  this  substance  produces 
a  great  increase  of  the  steady  cold  impulse  frequency. 
Further  studies  of  Dodt  el  al.  (21)  of  the  effect  of 
menthol  on  single  fibers  showed  that  menthol  exerts 
an  effect,  not  only  on  the  cold  fiber  activity  in  the 
usual  temperature  range  between  about  10°  and  38°C, 
but  also  on  the  paradoxical  cold  fiber  discharge 
between  45°  and  5o°C.  In  agreement  with  Gold- 
scheider  these  authors  observed  that  inenthol  sensi- 
tizes the  warm  fibers  also. 

The  effect  of  menthol  on  the  cold  and  warm  fibers 
can  be  completely  compensated  for  by  sudden  heating 
and  cooling,  respectively,  or  by  keeping  the  tongue 
at  a  constant  higher  and  lower  temperature,  respec- 
tively. These  measures  can  cause  the  cold  and  warm 
impulses  provoked  by  menthol  to  disappear  entirely. 
Thus  it  is  not  simply  the  question  of  a  chemical 
'inadequate'  stimulation  of  the  thermal  receptors  but 
of  a  sensitization  of  the  thermal  effect.  The  threshold 
of  the  menthol  effect  lies  between  the  concentrations 
of  I  :  1 ,000,000  and  1 :  500,000. 

Following  the  finding  of  Bing  &  Skouby  (8)  that 
the    introduction    of   small    amounts    of   cholinergic 


THEORETICAL    CONSIDERATIONS 


Cenlral  Threshold 


From  the  sensory  physiological  studies  it  appeared 
that  three  factors  are  governing  the  occurrence  of  a 
thermal  sensation:  a)  the  absolute  intracutaneous 
temperature,  d,  6)  the  rate  of  change  of  the  intra- 
cutaneous temperature,  dd/dl,  and  0  the  area  F,  the 
extension  of  the  stimulated  field. 

So  far  as  the  conditions  for  the  occurrence  of  a 
thermal  sensation  can  jje  expressed  in  physical- 
thermal  terms,  the  excitation  mechanism  can  be  repre- 
sented by  a  three-dimensional  system  of  thermal-spa- 
tial-temporal factors  which  arc  mutually  dependent 
on  each  other  and  to  a  great  extent  exchangeable. 
The  threshold  condition  can  thus  be  expressed  as  fol- 
lows: 

E  3  i(e,dS/dt,  F) 

where  E  is  the  abstraction  class  of  the  sensation,  3 
the  implication  sign  of  a  probability  implication  (75). 
A  sensation  of  cold,  for  example,  would  thus  occur 
when  a)  d  is  low,  b~)  the  rate  of  cooling,  dO/dl,  is 
sufficient,  and  c)  the  receptive  field  has  a  certain  area 

(42). 

The  recordings  of  the  action  potential  from  periph- 
eral cold  fibers  show  that  the  total  number,  n,  of  im- 


456 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


pulses  which  in  the  lime,  /,  arrive  at  the  central  organ 
also  is  a  function  of  these  three  factors, 

^3  0(e,de/d/,F) 

i.e.  the  value  of  n/t  becomes  greater  when  a)  the 
temperature  is  lower,  A)  the  rate  of  cooling  is  greater 
and  c)  when  the  receptive  field  is  enlarged — i.e.  when 
the  number  of  stimulated  cold  receptors  is  increased. 
The  rate  of  «//  is  nothing  else  but  the  central  threshold 
which  thus  can  be  written, 


E3^ 


c 


The  results  of  the  sen.sory-physiological  studies  are 
thus  in  very  good  accordance  with  those  obtained 
from  electrophysiological  investigations  on  the  specific 
thermal  fibers. 

The  declining  impulse  frequency  at  constant  tem- 
perature is  the  so-called  "physiological  adaptation'  of 
the  thermal  sense  recorded  objectively.  As  Hensel 
(42)  concluded  from  his  sensory-physiological  studies, 
it  should  be  more  correct  to  avoid  the  use  of  the  partic- 
ular words,  adaptation  or  change  of  excitability  in 
order  to  express  the  temporal  decrease  of  the  excita- 
tion under  a  constant  stimulus,  as  these  expressions 
lead  to  a  conception  of  a  specific  process  separated 
from  excitation.  Adaptation  is  then  assumed  when  a 
temporal  change  of  the  excitation  occurs  while  the 
stimulus  is  kept  constant.  But  this  depends  upon  the 
definition  of  stimulation.  When  as  in  the  thermal 
sense  the  temperature  (9)  is  the  stimulus,  adaptation 
appears  at  constant  stimulation.  If,  however,  the 
stimulus  is  the  rate  of  temperature  change,  dS  d<, 
there  will  be  no  adaptation  during  constant  stimula- 
tion. According  to  the  usual  definition,  adaptation  is 
therefore  nothing  else  than  an  indirect  description  of 
the  time  factor  of  a  sense  organ  based  on  its  response 
to  a  specific  mode  of  stimulation. 

At  constant  temperature  of  the  skin,  the  magnitude 
of  nit  is  dependent  upon  the  temperature  and  the 
area  of  the  skin.  If  the  thermal  receptors  were  evenly 
distributed,  the  thermosensible  tonus  would  thus  be  a 
direct  function  of  the  integral  skin  temperature.  This 
is,  however,  not  the  case  as  some  parts,  especially  the 
trigeminal  area,  display  a  much  greater  density  of 
thermal  receptors  and  are  thus  likely  to  exert  a  more 
dominant  influence  upon  the  thermoregulation  of  the 
body.  It  is  very  likely  that  the  central  threshold  of 
conscious  cold  sensations  lies  at  a  higher  level  than  the 
threshold  of  the  thermal  receptor  discharge  (34,  54), 


which  implies  that  a  certain  part  of  the  afferent 
thermoregulatory  inflow  occurs  below  the  threshold 
of  our  consciousness. 

Excitation  Alecfianism  of  1  hernial  Receptors 

The  fact  that  there  is  a  distinct  discharge  of  im- 
pulses from  thermal  receptors  when  there  is  a  com- 
plete temperature  equilibrium  between  the  two  sides 
of  the  receptor  layer,  i.e.  when  the  spatial  as  well  as 
the  temporal  temperature  gradient  is  zero,  shows  that 
this  activity  does  not  depend  upon  any  exchange  of 
thermal  energy.  Thus  there  must  occur  in  the  recep- 
tors, processes — probably  of  a  chemical  nature — 
which  are  governed  by  temperature  without  any 
external  exchange  of  energy  in  the  skin. 

For  this  reason  it  is  not  practicable  to  express  the 
thresholds  of  the  temperature  sense — in  analogy  with 
the  eye  and  the  ear — in  terms  of  a  thermal  energy. 

The  course  of  the  receptor  discharge  at  constant 
temperatures  and  particularly  the  effect  of  tempera- 
ture changes  suggests  that  we  have  to  deal  with  at 
least  two  interacting  processes,  one  exciting  and  one 
inhibiting.  We  can  thus,  according  to  Sand  (77), 
assume  that  the  frequency  of  the  steady  discharge  of 
the  cold  receptor,  n,  is  dependent  upon  the  difference 
between  two  temperature  dependent  processes,  E  and 
/.  The  difference  between  these  should  give  the  im- 
pulse frequency,  n  (45,  54). 


FIG.  26.  Graphs  illustrating  discharging  mechanism  of  a 
cold  receptor.  Abscissae,  skin  temperatures;  ordinates,  rates  of 
impulse  discharge.  In  the  lower  left  is  a  plot  of  the  steady 
discharge  of  a  cold  receptor  assuming  that  the  frequency  of  dis- 
charge (n)  is  a  function  of  the  difference  between  two  tempera- 
ture dependent  processes  E  and  7  (jibove').  On  the  right  is  illus- 
trated the  time  course  of  the  effect  of  sudden  cooling  from  a 
temperature  of  fli  to  one  of  ^2  and  back  to  ffi.  The  intersection  of 
the  curves  of  E  and  /  gives  the  upper  threshold  temperature 
9o  of  the  cold  receptor.  [From  Zotterman  (100).] 


THERMAL   SENSATIONS 


457 


These  functions  E  and  /  seem  to  resemble  exponen- 
tial functions  with  different  constants  as  follows: 


When  the  temperature  6  =  6,,,  n  =  o.  l(  a  <  l3, 
we  obtain  curves  as  shown  in  figure  26  where  the 
difference  curve,  n,  resembles  the  experimental  curves 
of  the  steady  discharge  obtained  from  single  cold 
fibers. 

In  the  above  equation  only  the  temperature  de- 
pendence has  been  considered.  The  time  dependence 
can,  however,  be  included  in  the  equation  in  a  manner 


which  rather  closely  describes  the  "adaptive'  part  of 
the  response  of  thermal  receptors,  i.e.  the  response  to 
sudden  temperature  changes.  The  equation  can  be 

written: 

n  =  A[(a,  -  e--")£  -  (6,  -  e"'^')/]- 

The  constants  are  dependent  upon  the  previous 
excitatory  level  of  the  thermal  receptor.  Figure  26 
shows  how  these  functions  can  be  used  to  predict  the 
behavior  of  a  cold  receptor  at  a  sudden  drop  in  tem- 
perature from  di  to  d-i  and  back  again.  For  further 
details  and  the  computation  of  the  constants  see 
Hensel  (45). 


REFERENCES 


1.  Alrutz,  S.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  7  :  32  i ,  1897. 

2.  Alrutz,  S.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  10:  340,  1900. 

3.  Bailey,  R.  A.  and  P.  Glees.  J.  Physiol.  1 13:  37P,  1951. 

4.  Bazett,  H.  C.  and  B.  McGlone.  A.M. A    Arch.  Neurol. & 
Psychiat.  27:  1031,  1932. 

5.  Bazett,  H.  C,  B.  McGlone,  R.  G.  Williams  and  H.  M. 
Lufkin.  A.M.A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  27:  489,  1932. 

6.  Beetz,  F.  Arch.  Gyndk.  162:  106,  1936. 

7.  Bernhard,  C.  G.  and  R.  Gr.'\nit  J.  Gen.  Physiol   29:  257, 
1946. 

8.  Bing,  H.  J.  and  A.  P.  .Skoubv.  .Ida  physiol.  scandinav.  21  : 
286,  1950. 

9.  Blix,  M.  Uppsala  lakaref.  forh.  18:  87,  1882. 

10.  Blix,  M.  ^Ischr.  Biol.  20:  141,  1884. 

1 1 .  Bohnenkamp.  H.  and  \V.  Pasquai.  Deutsche  ^tschr.  .Nervenh. 
126:  138,  1932. 

12.  Bollock,  T.  H.  and  R.  B.  Cowles.  Science  1 15:  541,  1952. 

13.  Bullock.  T.  H.  and  T.  P.  J.  Diecke.  J.  Physiol.  134:  47, 

1956- 

14.  Bullock,  T.  H.  and  D.  A.  Faulstick.  Fed.  Proc.  12:  22, 

1953- 

15.  Cohen,  M.  J.,  S.  Landgren,  L.  Strom  and  Y.  Zotter- 
MAN.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  Suppi.   135,  1957. 

16.  Gushing,  H.  Brain  32:  44,  1909. 

17.  DoDT,  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  27:  295,  1952. 

18.  DoDT,  E.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  29:  91,  1953. 

19.  DoDT,  E.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  31:  89,  1954. 

20.  DoDT,  E.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  263:  188,  1956. 

21.  DoDT,  E.,  A.  P.  Skouby  and  Y.  Zotterman.  .ida  physiol. 
scandinav.  28:  loi,  1953. 

22.  DoDT,  E.  .■\ND  Y.  Zotterman.  .Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26; 

238,  195^- 

23.  Dodt,  E.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26. 

345.  '952- 

24.  Dodt,  E.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  26: 

358,  195'^- 

25.  Ebaugh,  F.  G.,  Jr.  and  R.  Thauer.  J".  Appl.  Physiol.  3: 

173.  1950- 

26.  Ebbecke,  U.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  169:  395,  1917. 

27.  Endres,  G.  Z'schr.  Biol.  89:  536,  1930. 

28.  Gerard,  M.  W.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  9:  306, 
1923. 


29.  Gernandt,  B.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta,  psychul.  scandinav. 
1 1  :  248,  1946. 

30.  Gertz,  E.  ^tschr.  Sinnesphysiol.  52:  1,  1 921. 

31.  GlLSB.\CH,  C.  Die  Wirkung  grossjidchiger   Temperalurrei.ze  von 
verschiedener  Stcilheit  (Dissertation).  Heidelberg,  1 949. 

32.  Goldscheider,  a.  In :  Handhuch  der  norrnalen  und pathologischen 
Physiologie  yil:  131,  1926. 

33.  Goldscheider,  A.  and  H.  Hahn.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  206: 

337>  1924- 

34.  Grant,  R.  T.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  13:  75,  1951. 

35.  Hacker,  F.  ^tschr.  Biol.  61 :  231,  191 3. 

36.  HXGcqviST,  G.  Z^schr.  mikroskop.-anat.  Forsch.  39:  3,  1936. 

37.  Hahn,  H.  Beitrdge  zur  Reizphysiologie.  Heidelberg:  Scherer, 
1949. 

38.  Hardy,  J.  D.  and  T.  W.  Oppel.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  17:  771, 
1938. 

39.  Hauer,  P.  y^tschr.  Biol.  85:  265,  1926. 

40.  Head,  H.  Brain  41 :  57,  1918. 

41.  Heilbrun,  W.  Deutsche  ^tschr.  Nervenh.  loi  :  290,  1928. 

42.  Hensel,  H.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  252:  165,  1950. 

43.  Hensel,  H.  ^tschr.  ges.  exper.  Med.  117:  587,  1951. 

44.  Hensel,  H.  ^Ischr.  Kreislaufforsch.  41  :  251,  1952. 

45.  Hensel,  H.  Ergebn.  Physiol.  47:  165,  1952. 

46.  Hensel,  H.  Arch,  ges.  Physiol.  256:  195,  1952. 

47.  Hensel,  H.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  257;  371,  1953. 

48.  Hensel,  H.  Acta  physiol.  .scandinav.  29:  109,  1953. 

49.  Hensel,  H.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  37:  509,  1955. 

50.  Hensel,  H.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  263:  48,  1956. 

51.  Hensel.  H.,  L.  Strom  and  Y.  Zotterman.  J.  .Neurophysiol. 

'4:423.  igai- 

52.  Hensel,  H.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  22: 

96.  1951- 

53.  Hensel,  H.  .\no  Y.  Zotterman.  J'.  Physiol.  115:  16,  1951. 

54.  Hensel,  H.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  23: 

29i>  ■951- 

55.  Hensel,  H.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  J.  .Neurophysiol .  14:  377, 

>95'- 

56.  Hensel,  H.  .\nd  Y.  Zotterman.  .Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  24: 

27.  1951- 

57.  Herget,  C.  M.,  L.  p.  GranaT!!  and  J.  D.  Hardy.  Am. 

J.  Physiol.  135:  20,  1 94 1. 


458 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


58.  Hering,  E.  In:  Herrmanns  Handbuch  der  Pbysiologie.  Ill, 
Pt.  2:  415,  1880. 

59.  HiRSCH,  L.  AND  H.  ScHRiEVER.  ^isclir.  liinl.  8g ;  1 ,  1930. 

60.  Holm,  K.  G.  Skmidinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  14:  249,  1903. 

61.  Kaila,  E.  Handbuch  der  Psychologic.  Basel:  Schwabe,  1951. 

62.  KiESOW,  F.  Arch.  Psychol.  22;  50,  191 1. 

63.  Landgren,  S.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  40;  202,  1957. 

64.  Lehmann,  a.  Die  Hauptgesetzc  des  Menschlichen  GefUhlsle 
bens.  Leipzig:  Reisland,  [892. 

65.  Lele,  p.  p.,  G.  VVeddell  and  C.  M.  Williams.  J.  Physiol. 
126:  206,  1954. 

66.  Lewis,  Th.,  G.  \V.  Pickering  and  P.  Rothschild.  Heart 
16:  I,  1931. 

67.  Marechaux,  E.  W.  and  K.  E.  Schafer.  .irch.  gcs.  Phyuol. 

251  :  765.  1949- 

68.  Maruhashi,  J.,  K.  Mizuguchi  and  I.  Tasaki.  J.  Physiol. 
117:  129,  1952. 

6q.  MouNTCASTLE,  V.  B.  and  E.  Henneman.  J.  Neurophystol . 
12:85,  ■949- 

70.  Noble,  G.  K.  and  A.  Schmidt.  Proc.  Am.  Philos.  .S'oc.  77: 
263,  1937. 

71.  Penfield,  W.  and  a.  T.  Rasmussen.  The  Cerebral  Cortex  in 
Man:  A  Clinical  Study  of  Localization  of  Function.  New  York : 
Macmillan,  1950. 

72.  Rein,  H.  ^tschr.  Biol.  82:  189,  1925. 

73.  Rein,  H.  and  H.  Strughold.  ^tschr.  Biol.  82:  553,  1925. 

74.  Rein,  H.  and  H.  Strughold.  ^tschr.  Biol.  87  :  599,  1928. 

75.  RENqvisT-REENPAA,  Y.  .illgemeinc  Sinnesphysiologie.  Vienna : 
Springer,  1936. 

76.  RucH,  T.  C.  In:  A  Textbook  of  Physiologviiythed.^,  edited 
by  J.  F.  Fulton.  Philadelphia:  Saunders,  1955. 

77.  Sand,  A.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  125:  524,  1938. 


78.  Schriever,  H.  and  H.  Strughold.  ^Ischr.  Binl.  84:  193, 
1926. 

79.  SjoQVIST,  O.  Acta  psychial.  et  neurol.  Suppl.  17,  1938. 

80.  Skoubv,  a.  p.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  24:  174,  1951. 

81.  Speiser,  M.  Arch.  Gynak.  146:  137,  1931. 

82.  Stein,  J.  and  V.   v.  Weizsacker.  Deutsche  .^tschr.  .\ervenh. 

99:  1.  '927- 

83.  Strughold,  H.   Verhandl.  phys.-med.  Gesellsch.    ]Viir.zb.  51: 
31,  1926. 

84.  Strughold,   H.   and   M.   Karbe.  ^tschr.   Biol.   83:    189, 

'925- 

85.  Strughold,  H.  and  R.  Porz.  ^tschr.  Biol.  91  :  563,  1931. 

86.  Strumpell,  a.  Deutsches  Arch.  klin.  .Med.  28:  43,  1881. 

87.  Thunberg,  T.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  1 1  :  382,  1901. 

88.  Thunberg,  T.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  12:  394,  1902. 

89.  Thunberg,  T.   In:  Handbuch  der  Physiologic  des  Menschen, 
edited  by  W.  Nagel.  Ill:  669,  1905. 

90.  Trotter,  W.  and  H.  M.  Davies.  J.  Physiol.  38:  134,  1909. 
gi.  von  Frey,  M.  Ber.  sdclis.  Gesellsch.  VViss.  47:  166,  1895. 

92.  VON  Skramlik,  E.  Arch.  Psychol.  4:  244,  1937. 

93.  Walker,  A.  E.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol. &  Psychiat.  43:  284, 
1940. 

94.  Weber,  E.  H.  Wagner's  Handwrirterbuch  der  Physiologic.  Ill, 
Pt.  2:481,  1846. 

95.  Zotterman,  Y.  Acta  med.  scandinav.  80:  185,  1933. 

96.  Zotterman,  Y.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  75:  106,  1936. 

97.  Zotterman,  Y.  J.  Physiol.  95:  i,  1939. 

98.  Zotterman,  Y.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  18:  181,  1949. 

99.  Zotterman,  Y.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  15:  357,  1953. 

100.   Zotterman,  Y.   In:  Fourth  Conference  on  the  .Verve  Impulse, 
edited  by  D.  Nachmansohn.  New  York:  Macy,  1954. 


CHAPTER    XIX 


Pain 


WILLIAM   H.   SWEET      j      Department  oj  Surgery,  Harvard  Medical  School,  Boston,  Massachusetts 


CHAPTER    C:ONTENT,S 

Pain   as   a   Sensation   with    Its   Own    Peripheral    and    Central 

Nervous  Apparatus 
Stimulus,  Sensation,  and  Their  Measurement 

Mechanical  Stimuli 

Correlation  with  Tissue  Damage 

Heat 

Electricity 

Distention  of  Viscera 

Arterial  Constriction  with  Ischemia  and  Arterial  Dilatation 

Inflammation 

Quantitation  of  Severity  of  Pain 
Animal   Versus   Human   Subjects   in   Pain   Studies 
End  Organs  for  Pain 

Normal  Skin 

Cornea 

Abnormal  Skin 

Special  Cutaneous  Sensory  Endings 

Deeper  Somatic  and  Visceral  Receptors 
Terminal  Sensory  Plexuses 
Peripheral  Sensory  Nerve  Fibers 

Single  Fiber  Studies 

Fiber  Diameters  and  Pain  Conduction 

Double  Pain  Responses  or  Second  Pain 
Pain   in   Abnormal   Anatomical   States  at   Periphery 

Division  of  Cutaneous  Nerves 

Hyperalgesic  State  After  Trauma 
Chemical  Excitants  of  Pain 
Posterior  and  Anterior  Roots 
Pain  and  Autonomic  Nervous  System 

Sympathetic  Nerv^es 

Parasympathetic  Nerves 
Spinal  Cord 
Medulla  Oblongata 
Mesencephalon 
Thalamus 
Cerebral  Hemispheres 

Stimulation 

Lesions 

Evoked  Potentials 

Second  Sensory  Area  in  Man 

Reaction  to  Pain 
Indifference  to  pain 


Pain  asymbolia 

Reactions    after    operations    on    frontal    lobes 
Conclusion 
Endocrines  and  Pain 
Itching  and  Tickling 
Pain  and  Inhibition 
Referred  Pain 


THE  NATURE  AND  RANGE  of  the  sensations  covered  lay 
the  word  '  pain'  elude  precise  definition.  Aristotle  (8) 
equated  pain  with  unpleasantness  whether  arising 
from  outside  the  body,  within  the  body  or  within  the 
'  soul'  (as  when  one  feels  miserable).  '  Pain  or  un- 
pleasantness' stood  for  him  as  the  opposite  to 'pleasure' 
and  he  considered  every  action  to  be  "accompanied 
by  pleasure  and  pain."  For  Spinoza  (65)  pain  was  a 
focal  form  of  sorrow  which  he  called  one  of  the  three 
primary  emotions.  Pain,  which  he  thought  of  as  the 
emotion  opposite  to  "pleasurable  excitment,"  he 
"related  to  a  man  when  one  of  his  parts  is  affected 
more  than  the  others;  melancholy,  on  the  other  hand, 
when  all  parts  are  equally  afl^'ected."  As  scientists  now 
tend  to  u.se  the  word,  '  pain'  contains  the  Spinozistic 
implication  that  the  unpleasant  feeling  is  specifically 
referred  to  some  place  or  places  in  the  body.  In  any 
case  it  is  this  more  localized  kind  of  pain  which  is 
more  amenable  to  physiologic  study  in  contradis- 
tinction to  diffuse  states  of  unpleasantness. 

But  from  the  standpoint  of  the  physician  it  is  neces- 
sary to  analyze  and  treat  every  type  of  disagreeable 
feeling  of  which  people  complain.  It  matters  not 
whether  the  individual  tags  it  by  the  label  'pain'.  Thus 
in  the  area  of  the  face  which  has  undergone  trigeminal 
denervation  about  5  per  cent  of  the  patients  have 
severe  annoying  sensations  which  they  may  call 
aches,  but  often  they  are  at  a  loss  for  words  to  describe 


459 


460 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  peculiar  sensation  so  extraordinary  and  un- 
matched by  any  of  their  previous  experiences.  Vet 
many  so  afflicted  find  the  feelings  sufficiently  intoler- 
able to  seek  major  surgery  for  relief  Certainly  such 
sensations  are  reasonably  classified  as  an  unusual  form 
of  pain,  and  the  elucidation  of  the  mechanism  of 
severe  unpleasantness  referred  to  an  anesthetic  area  in 
the  presumed  absence  of  an  organic  central  lesion  re- 
mains one  of  many  challenE^es  to  the  neurophysi- 
ologist. 

Pain  may  be  arbitrarily  divided  into  two  main 
elements,  the  initial  sensation  and  the  reaction  to  that 
sensation.  As  Beecher  (15)  has  emphasized,  the  signifi- 
cance of  the  pain  to  the  individual  plays  a  major  role 
in  determining  the  extent  of  the  second  reactive  com- 
ponent of  the  feeling.  Thus  he  found  that  only  32  per 
cent  of  1 50  war-injured  men  had  pain  severe  enough  to 
require  a  narcotic,  whereas  83  per  cent  of  150  male 
civilians  undergoing  surgery  involving  much  less 
trauma  required  narcotics.  For  the  soldier  the  war 
wound  marked  the  end  of  a  gravely  hazardous  form  of 
life,  whereas  no  such  compensation  and,  at  worst, 
serious  problems  beset  the  operated  civilian.  Beecher 
interpreted  the  differing  degree  of  complaint  in  these 
two  settings  as  indicating  "that  the  reaction  or 
processing  phase  is  very  often  of  more  importance  in 
suffering  than  is  the  original  sensation."  Indeed  the 
original  sensation  as  well  may  appear  as  a  symptom 
of  protest,  as  from  the  person  who  develops  a  '  sick 
headache'  at  will;  or  the  pain  may  come  as  a  conse- 
quence of  previous  conditioning  as  in  'a  painful 
memory.'  Contrariwise,  various  cerebral  deficiency 
states  result  in  a  reduced  reaction  to  afferent  impulses 
for  pain  which  would  normally  evoke  a  lively  response. 

In  this  essay  we  shall  devote  most  of  the  discussion 
to  the  sensation  of  pain  as  evoked  by  specific  stimuli 
to  sensory  endings  or  pathways,  recognizing,  how- 
ever, the  vast  importance  of  the  psychological  com- 
ponent of  the  response  and  the  virtual  impossibility  of 
separating  this  from  the  primary  awareness  of  pain. 


PAIN     AS    A    SENSATION     WITH     ITS    OWN     CENTRAL 
AND   PERIPHERAL  APPARATUS 

The  validity  of  searching  for  a  special  sensory 
mechanism  concerned  wholly  or  mainly  with  pain 
requires  inquiry.  The  physiologist  has  not  sought  out 
specific  nervous  pathways  subserving  pleasure,  the 
philosopher's  antipode  to  pain;  is  it  sensible  to  look 
for  pain  pathways?  One  can  answer  promptly  that  it 
is  and  has  proved  eminently  fruitful  to  do  so  because 


certain  stimuli  to  certain  areas  almost  invariaiilv 
bring  on  pain  in  man,  whereas  the  same  constancy  of 
relationship  in  no  way  applies  to  pleasure.  One  may 
venture  to  state  that  even  the  amorous  male  with  the 
most  Ca.sanovian  success  has  not  developed  a  form  and 
site  of  stimulus  which  constantly  evokes  pleasure  in 
his  partner,  thous^h  in  the  absence  of  a  criticallv  re- 
ported series  this  can  be  no  more  than  the  author's 
disgruntled  surmi.se. 

Erasmus  Darwin (53,  pp.  121  and  125)  thought  pain 
to  be  the  consequence  of  any  excessive  stimulation  and 
a  result  of  exaggeration  of  sensations  of  heat,  touch, 
sight,  taste  or  smell.  This  intensive  theory  of  pain  in 
one  modification  or  another  has  found  many  sup- 
porters. And  we  can  scarcely  disagree  with  William 
James'  (134)  conclusion  that  it  is  certain  that  sensa- 
tions of  every  order  which  in  moderate  degree  are 
rather  pleasant  than  otherwise  become  unpleasant 
when  their  intensity  grows  too  strong.  For  example,  in 
1934  Nafe  (198)  drew  attention  to  the  fact  that  when 
smooth  muscle  was  in  spastic  contraction  at  the 
extremes  of  heat  and  cold,  52°C  and  3°C  respectively, 
there  was  pain.  At  levels  intermediate  between  these 
there  was  only  a  sense  of  warmth  or  coolness.  As 
stimulation  became  more  intense  with  a  rise  in  tem- 
perature the  quality  of  the  sensation  was  altered  from 
warmth,  to  heat,  to  pain;  all  mediated  he  thought  bs- 
the  same  peripheral  equipment  and  integrated  at  the 
thalamocortical  level.  More  recently  Gooddy  (104) 
has  argued  that  "any  nervous  pathways  are  potential 
pain  pathways,"  i.e.  that  any  pathway  may  provide 
"the  impulse  patterns  that  are  associated  with  the 
perception  of  pain.  "  In  certain  patients  successive 
operations  on  peripheral  nerves,  posterior  roots,  spinal 
cord,  thalamus  and  cerebral  hemispheres  may  all  fail 
to  give  permanent  relief  from  pain.  From  such  series 
of  events,  infrequent  though  they  are,  Gooddy  reaches 
the  extreme  point  of  view  that  "unless  the  whole 
nervous  system  is  destroyed,  the  abnormal  patterns 
(evoking  pain)  gradually  establish  themselves  anew. 

The  most  clear-cut  evidence  to  the  contrary,  that 
at  least  some  pain  is  to  be  regarded  as  a  particular 
form  of  sensation  with  its  own  pathways  and  not 
merely  an  intensification  of  other  forms,  is  provided 
by  patients  with  a  lesion  confined  to  the  anterior 
quadrant  of  the  spinal  cord.  This  usually  deprives 
them  of  the  capacity  to  feel  pain  in  response  to  a  wide 
variety  of  noxa  previously  painful,  yet  proprioceptive 
and  light  touch  sensibility  are  virtually  unimpaired. 
This  is  the  typical  finding  after  the  operation  of 
anterolateral   cordotomx.    Although   thermanesthesia 


PAIN 


461 


is  usually  present  along  with  analgesia,  a  rare  patient 
after  such  a  cordotomy  may  show  only  the  former 
without  the  latter  or  vice  versa,  indicating  that  there 
are  special  pathways  for  pain  and  others  for  tempera- 
ture sensation  which  are  nearly,  but  not  precisely,  co- 
extensive (296,  p.  259).  Schiff  (238)  one  century  ago 
made  the  fundamental  observation  that  lesions  of  the 
spinal  cord  in  rabbits,  sparing  only  the  posterior 
columns,  resulted  in  animals  which  would  make  a 
number  of  responses  to  touch  whereas  they  would  ig- 
nore presumably  painful  deep  stimuli.  He  recognized 
the  similarity  between  this  state  and  the  clinical  con- 
dition of  analgesia  without  anesthesia  to  touch,  de- 
scribed in  man  both  by  Beau  and  by  Vieusseux  [  cited 
by  Schiff  (238),  p.  253]. 

Such  data,  though,  do  not  prove  that  impulses  for 
pain  and  touch  may  not  use  the  same  fibers  in  pe- 
ripheral nerves  and  there  has  been  no  histologic  cor- 
relation between  lesions  of  certain  types  of  peripheral 
nerve  fiber  and  a  disassociated  loss  of  touch  or  pain. 
Although  such  loss  may  occur  in  leprosy — the  Bacillus 
leprae  typically  attacks  only  the  peripheral  and  not 
the  central  nervous  system — a  focal  degeneration  of 
dorsal  funiculi  has  also  been  seen  in  this  disorder  by 
Wilson  (301,  fig.  92,  p.  753).  Hence  a  purely  periph- 
eral lesion  may  not  be  taken  for  granted  here  as  the 
explanation  of  a  loss  of  pain  without  touch  or  the 
reverse.  However,  conduction  by  peripheral  nerves 
can  become  impaired  in  such  fashion  that  a  differ- 
ential loss  of  various  forms  of  sensation  occurs.  Thus 
Herzen  (127)  was  the  first  to  note  that  pressure  on  a 
human  peripheral  nerve,  the  sciatic,  caused  initially 
loss  of  touch  sensibility,  shortly  thereafter  that  of  cold, 
much  later  that  of  warmth  and  finally  of  pain.  Gold- 
scheider  (102)  in  the  same  year  likewise  observed  a 
differential  loss  of  sensory  modalities,  although  in  a 
different  order,  when  a  branch  of  a  peripheral  nerve 
was  cocainized.  He  thought  cold  was  blocked  first, 
then,  in  sequence,  warmth,  pain  and  pressure.  Modifi- 
cations of  the  first  method  of  compression  or  asphvxia 
of  the  nerve  and  of  the  second,  pharmacologic  block, 
have  since  been  used  extensively  in  a  study  of  the 
specificity  of  nerve  fibers  for  single  modalities  of 
sensation.  Unequi\ocal  proof  that  one  peripheral 
fiber  is  devoted  to  but  one  type  of  sensory  modality, 
pain,  touch,  cold  or  warmth  has  not  been  advanced 
as  yet.  The  evidence  bearing  on  this  and  on  the  ques- 
tion of  special  sensory  end  organs  for  pain  will  be  pre- 
sented later.  Before  studying  the  nervous  system  itself 
we  may  properly  consider  the  tactics  used  in  arousing 
its  responses. 


STIMULUS,  SENSATION  AND  THEIR  MEASUREMENT 

Mechanical  Stimuli 

Quantitative  assessment  of  pain  involved,  of  course, 
measurement  both  of  stimulus  and  sensation.  In  the 
earliest  tactics  one  pricked  the  surface  to  be  tested 
with  needle  points  mounted  either  in  fibers  which 
bent  at  a  calibrated  force  or  on  a  calibrated  spring — a 
method  which  remains  the  best  for  many  clinical 
physiological  studies. 

Correlation  with  Tissue  Damage 

The  adequate  stimulus  for  pain,  whether  it  is  me- 
chanical, thermal,  electrical  or  chemical,  is  poten- 
tially or  actually  productive  of  tissue  damage.  Hence, 
the  immediate  zone  of  reception  on  which  the  stim- 
ulus is  acting  soon  becomes  modified  in  serial  determi- 
nations at  the  same  site.  Thus  Lewis  (171,  p.  106) 
pointed  out  that,  if  the  skin  of  the  front  of  the  forearm 
is  pricked  with  a  needlejust  hard  enough  to  cause  pain, 
most  ofthe.se  pricks  will  subsequently  show  signs  of  tis- 
sue damage  in  the  form  of  little  circles  of  redness.  Ther- 
mal radiation  in  order  to  evoke  pain  requires  an  en- 
ergy (expressed  in  millicalories  per  second  per  square 
centimeter)  which  is  2000  times  that  of  the  threshold 
for  warmth.  In  fact.  Hardy  et  al.  (118,  pp.  23,  53), 
the  workei-s  responsible  for  these  figures,  state  that  the 
thermal  radiation  threshold  for  '  pricking  pain'  lies  at 
a  skin  temperature  of  roughly  45°C,  which  is  likewise 
the  threshold  temperature  range  for  the  production  of 
skin  damage,  according  to  Moritz  &  Henriques  (192). 
In  agreement  with  this  Benjamin  (18)  finds  the 
threshold  for  the  production  of  a  cutaneous  flare  by 
heat  is  very  clo.se  to  the  pain  threshold. 

Heat 

Nevertheless  thermal  radiation  which  eliminates 
simultaneous  contact  and  pressure  sensations  has 
formed  the  basis  for  much  of  the  modern  work  on  pain 
thresholds  since  the  description  by  Hardy  et  al.  (115) 
of  their 'dolorimeter.'  This  apparatus  permits  control 
of  the  intensity  and  duration  of  applied  heat  and  its 
measurement  by  a  radiometer.  With  critical,  careful 
use  of  this  instrument  so  arranged  as  to  provide  a 
radiation  time  of  3  sec,  it  is  their  contention  that  the 
pain  threshold  is  constant  from  person  to  person  and 
in  the  same  individual  from  time  to  time.  The  three  in- 
vestigators were  the  initial  subjects  and  they  studied 
themselves  nearly  every  day  for  almost  a  year,  di- 


462 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


reeling  the  heat  to  an  area  of  the  forehead  thoroughly 
blackened  with  India  ink  and  obtaining  values  which 
all  fell  within  ±  1 2  per  cent  of  the  mean.  They  describe 
the  subject's  experience  as  follows:  "The  sensation  is 
one  of  warmth,  heat  and  burning  which  seems  to 
"swell'  and  then  to  "draw  together'  into  a  prick  at  the 
end  of  the  third  second.  Minimal  after-sensations  of 
heat  and  burning  pain  are  common."  This  they  call 
the  pricking  pain  threshold.  Bigelovv  et  al.  (26)  also 
identified  another  threshold — that  for  "burning  pain.' 
This  on  the  forehead  is,  they  say,  about  20  to  30  meal, 
lower  than  the  pricking  pain  threshold.  When  these 
workers  extended  their  measurements  to  1 50  individu- 
als about  the  same  mean  intensity  of  stimulus,  0.21  gm 
cal.  per  sec.  per  cm-,  evoked  '  pricking  pain'  with  a 
maximal  variation  of  ±  1 5  per  cent. 

This  uniformity  is  said  to  persist  throughout  a  24  hr. 
period  of  enforced  wakefulness  (241)  in  women  before, 
during  and  after  labor  (135),  and  over  much  of  the 
body  surface  (i  18).  But  Wolfl  &  Goodell  (304)  also 
state  that,  if  the  subject  is  unable  to  concentrate  on  the 
testing  procedure  or  "to  maintain  a  detached,  un- 
prejudiced attitude"  because  of  fatigue,  lethargy,  sug- 
gestibility or  other  reason,  then  the  pain  threshold 
varies  greatly  and  is  unpredictable. 

A  number  of  workers  have  been  unable  to  confirm 
the  described  uniformity  of  pain  threshold  in  these 
painfully  collected  data.  Chapman  &  Jones  (40) 
found  the  theshold  of  200  normal  subjects  to  vary 
much  more  widely — from  —40  per  cent  to  -I-50  per 
cent.  Others  who  have  reported  inconstancy  of 
thresholds  by  this  technique  include  Clausen  &  King 
(44),  Leduc  &  Slaughter  (160),  Schamp  &  Schamp 
(237)  and  Slaughter  &  Wright  (252).  Benjamin  (20) 
agreed  that  the  tactic  of  painting  the  skin  with  India 
ink  resulted  in  absorption  of  nearly  all  (94  per  cent) 
of  the  incident  heat  and  it^  transmission  into  the  skin 
by  conduction,  but  found  pain  sensitivits'  in  the  palm 
less  than  in  the  forearm.  Whyte  (297}  has  advanced 
this  cogent  criticism:  the  validity  of  the  method  de- 
pends on  the  contention  of  Oppel  &  Hardy  (203)  that 
the  rise  in  skin  temperature  produced  by  radiant  heat 
is  proportional  to  thermal  intensity.  If  this  were  true, 
then  at  the  increa.sed  radiant  heat  thresholds  for 
morphine  described  by  Wolff  ?<  al.  (306)  extrapolation 
of  their  curves  would  indicate  that  the  skin  tempera- 
tures would  have  reached  about  54°C.  The  actual  fore- 
head temperatures  of  Whyte's  subjects  at  the  pricking 
pain  threshold  were  about  the  same  before  and  after 
morphine,  ranging  from  46.2  to  47.5°C.  From  this 
Whyte  logically  concludes  that  an  increase  in  sweating 


or  in  blood  flow  to  the  skin  may  have  occurred  follow- 
ing the  morphine  rather  than  the  presumed  increase 
in  nervous  threshold.  Beecher  and  associates  have  also 
been  sharp  critics  of  the  contention  that  consistent 
thresholds  are  obtainable  by  the  method.  Denton  & 
Beecher  (63)  found  that  an  operator  widely  experi- 
enced in  the  radiant  heat  method,  who  was  called  in 
to  correct  their  failure  to  get  consistent  data,  was  able 
to  do  so  as  long  as  he  knew  what  drug  had  been  ad- 
ministered; he  failed  when  he  did  not  know.  They 
interpreted  this  to  be  a  consequence  of  unconscious 
guidance  by  the  operator  of  the  subjective  response  he 
was  seeking.  Much  space  has  been  devoted  to  con- 
sideration of  this  single  technique  in  order  to  point  out 
the  difhculties  of  precise  measurement  of  a  sensation 
as  threatening  to  the  individual  as  pain. 

Electricity 

Electrical  stimulation  has  also  been  used  to  test 
cutaneous  sensation.  The  threshold  feeling,  according 
to  Bishop  (27),  varies  depending  upon  the  special 
sensitivity  of  the  skin  spot  tested.  Using  a  condenser 
discharge  deli\'ering  a  spark  when  the  point  of  the 
stimulating  needle  was  about  0.5  mm  from  the  .skin, 
he  was  able  to  stimulate  without  mechanical  contact. 
With  this  device  he  found  spots  "mediating  ordinary 
touch  or  light  pressure"  and  others  inducing  the  sensa- 
tion of  prick  "which  becomes  pricking  pain  on  a 
stronger  stimulation."  But  he  says  that  a  "single 
stimulus  applied  to  a  single  prick  spot  at  the  threshold 
is  not  painful  but  elicits  a  tactile  experience  usually 
accompanied  by  a  faint  aura  of  itch.  This  tactile 
sensation  is  not  associated  with  a  feeling  of  pressure. 
.  .  .  On  the  other  hand  a  single  threshold  stimulus 
applied  to  a  touch  ending  is  experienced  as  a  slight 
tap."  Distinctions  between  a  tactile  sensation  with  and 
one  without  pressure,  and  between  a  prick  and  prick- 
ing pain,  certainly  do  not  lend  themselves  to  quantita- 
tive analysis.  Mueller  et  al.  (ii^i),  seeking  to  develop 
an  electrical  method  for  the  testing  of  pain  threshold, 
found  the  most  clear-cut  end  point  to  be  the  sensation 
of  prick  but,  in  critical  electrical  measurements,  they 
found  the  prick  to  occur  during  breakdown  of  skin 
impedance  .so  that  the  electrical  quantity  was  not 
purely  an  index  of  threshold  pain  but  was  dependent 
on  the  dielectric  properties  of  the  skin.  Beecher  (16) 
after  a  thorough  re\  iew  of  the  whole  proijlein  of  the 
measurement  of  pain  of  both  'experimental'  and 
'pathological'  types  concludes  that  "no  con\incing 
demonstration    has    yet    been    given    that    the    pain 


PAIN 


463 


threshold  is  a  constant  from  man  to  man  or  from  one 
time  to  another  in  a  given  man."  He  attributes  the 
variability  to  the 'psychic  reaction  component'  rather 
than  the 'original  sensation.'  Precise  determinations  of 
pain  threshold  find  perhaps  their  greatest  practical 
utility  in  the  pharmacologic  appraisal  of  analgesic 
agents — a  subject  beyond  our  scope.  To  me  it  appears 
that  firmer  conclusions  are  tenable  when  one  works 
with  stimuli  in  animals  and  man  which,  in  normal 
man,  consistenth'  evoke  unequivocal  and  unalloyed 
pain.  The  subsequent  discussion  will  seek  to  emphasize 
such  work. 

Other  methods  for  the  stud\'  of  pain  either  deliber- 
ately evoked  or  arising  in  pathological  states  will  now 
be  considered. 


Distention  of  Viscera 

Incision  into  and  passing  a  needle  through  skeletal 
mu.scle  are  almost  painless,  and  the  abdominal 
viscera  exposed  under  local  anesthesia  may  be  cut, 
torn  or  burned  as  long  as  the  parietal  peritoneum  and 
roots  of  the  mesentery  are  not  stimulated  (164).  These 
viscera  are  capable  of  initiating  impulses  for  pain  upon 
the  appropriate  stimulus,  however,  and  it  was  Len- 
nander  (165)  who  demonstrated  that  it  was  distension 
of  the  human  kidney  pelvis  which  was  painful.  Hurst 
(131)  extended  this  principle,  that  distension  is  the 
pain-evoking  stimulus  for  hollow  viscera,  to  studies  of 
the  alimentary  canal.  Davis  et  al.  (54,  56)  have  applied 
the  same  tactic  to  studies  of  the  gall  bladder.  Rapid 
expansion  of  the  capsule  of  solid  organs  like  the  li\er 
and  kidney  also  hurts. 


Arterial  Constriction  with  Ischemia  and  Arterial  Dilatation 

Both  arterial  constriction  or  occlusion  to  the  point 
of  ischemia  and  severe  arterial  dilatation  at  times 
associated  with  excessive  pulsation  in  a  part  are  pro- 
ductive of  pain.  Sutton  &  Lueth  (257)  thought  that 
myocardial  anoxia  was  the  physiological  stimulus  for 
cardiac  pain  when  they  found  that  lightly  anesthetized 
dogs  gave  responses  suggestive  of  pain  when  a  coronary 
artery  was  occluded  by  a  ligature.  Gorham  (107)  con- 
firmed these  results  and  added  the  ob.servation  that  if 
three  ligatures  were  placed  in  the  wall  of  the  coronary 
artery  so  that  divergent  traction  on  them  would  tend 
to  distend  the  vessel,  responses  of  'pain'  also  occurred. 
The  headache  of  migraine  is  one  of  the  better  studied 
examples  of  a  pain  probably  brought  on  by  arterial 


dilation.  Wolff,  a  prominent  exponent  of  this  view, 
summarized  the  e\idence  for  it  in  1948  C303,  pp.  265 
to  288).  Histamine  produces,  among  other  effects, 
painful  distension  of  arteries  and  presents  a  means  of 
evoking  headache  experimentally,  although  this 
differs  from  that  seen  in  migraine  in  at  least  eight 
respects  according  to  W'olff  (303,  pp.  289  to  290).  The 
reverse  situation,  ischemia  as  a  cause  of  pain,  is  seen 
both  in  intermittent  claudication  affecting  especially 
the  lower  limbs  and  in  cardiac  angina.  Lewis,  Picker- 
ing and  Rothschild  have  developed  a  testing  pro- 
cedure (171,  p.  97)  involving  voluntary  manual  grip- 
ping movements  at  the  rate  of  i  per  sec.  developing  a 
tension  of  20  to  28  lb.  Such  movements,  normally 
painless  for  many  minutes,  soon  cause  pain  if  the 
circulation  to  the  arm  is  stopped  by  inflation  of  a 
proximally  placed  cuff.  Lewis  (170)  initiated  and 
Kellgren  (140)  followed  with  another  type  of  test,  the 
injection  of  hypertonic  saline  into  muscles,  tendons, 
ligaments  and  joints  to  provide  and  permit  the  analysis 
of  pain  from  these  deeper  structures. 

Inflammation 

Inflammation,  arising  from  disease  or  produced 
experimentally,  is  apparently  another  process  whereby 
previously  painless  stimuli  appear  to  become  painful; 
this  is  true  for  skin  (i  72),  for  deeper  somatic  structures 
(150)  and  for  the  viscera  (142).  Thus  the  inflamed 
appendix  hurts  when  pinched,  but  not  the  normal 
appendix. 

Excellent  summaries  of  various  experimental 
methods  appear  in  Hardy  et  al.  (118,  Chapter  III) 
and  in  Beecher  (16,  Section  V);  Lewis  (171,  Chapter 
I)  gives  a  useful  catalogue  of  the  effective  stimuli  for 
each  of  the  pain-sensitive  tissues  of  the  body. 


Qjiantitation  0/  Seventy  of  Pain 

The  purely  subjective  character  of  pain  has  given 
rise  to  great  difficulty  in  efforts  at  quantitation,  but 
Hardy  et  al.  (118,  p.  156)  have  thought  that  trained 
observers  can  distinguish  as  many  as  2 1  different 
degrees  of  pain,  from  zero  to  maximum,  arising  from 
radiant  heat.  That  is  to  say,  there  were  21  steps  or 
'just  noticeable  differences  (jnd's)'  as  the  amount  of 
radiant  heat  was  increased.  They  suggested  a  unit  for 
pain  sensation,  a  '  dol'  equivalent  to  the  sum  of  2 
jnd's;  pain  of  ceiling  intensity  has  a  value  of  lo''^ 
'dols.'  Armstrong  et  al.  (9)  have  found  their  trained 


464 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSIGLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


group  able  to  distinguish  at  least  8,  possibly  up  to  16, 
units  of  pain  intensity  when  a  chemical  excitant  of 
pain  is  applied  to  the  exposed  base  of  a  canthardin 
blister. 


ANIMAL  VERSUS  HUMAN  SUBJECTS  IN  PAIN  STUDIES 

VVaterston  (284)  has  pointed  out  that  one's  natural 
repugnance  to  investigating  pain  in  man  must  be 
overcome  because  of  "  the  \alue  and  importance  .  .  . 
of  the  information  which  can  be  thus  obtained  and  by 
this  means  only."  The  final  and  conclusive  arbiter  on 
all  questions  relating  to  any  sensation  must  i^e  man 
experiencing  that  sensation  and  able  to  describe  it  in 
words;  this  is  particularly  true  of  pain  which  by  defi- 
nition must  have  .some  degree  of  affective  component 
of  unpleasantness.  I  should  agree  without  reservation 
with  Hardy  et  al.  (116,  p.  2)  that "  the  verbal  report  of 
the  instructed  subject  is  the  most  reliable  evidence  that 
the  pain  threshold  has  been  reached."  But  it  has 
seemed  logical  to  many  to  assume  that  a  maneuver 
which  consistently  evokes  pain  in  man  and  pro\okes 
in  him  some  form  of  motor  response  when  pain  is  felt 
may  Ije  used  in  animals  and  the  motor  response  of  the 
creature  taken  as  an  end  point  indicative  of  pain.  Un- 
fortunately this  conclusion  is  not  without  pitfalls. 
Such  reasoning  led  Gerard  (95,  p.  335)  to  make  a 
series  of  successively  more  rostral  incisions  into  the 
spinal  or  descending  trigeminal  tract  in  cats,  each 
incision  about  1.5  mm  rostral  to  the  previous  one.' 
She  began  at  the  midbulbar  level  at  the  obex  and 
ascended  until  stimulation  of  the  cornea  no  longer 
elicited  the  "  pain  reflexes'  of  struggling,  pupillary 
dilatation  and  rise  of  arterial  pressure.  Not  until  the 
cut  reached  the  midpontine  area  did  corneal  stimula- 
tion fail  to  evoke  such  reflexes,  .so  she  naturally  con- 
cluded that  the  pain  fibers  from  the  cornea  terminate 
just  below  this  point.  But  in  man  all  such  fibers  termi- 
nate much  lower,  in  fact  all  below  the  obex,  because 
cutting  of  all  of  the  descending  trigeminal  tract  at  this 
level  produces  enduring  analgesia  of  the  entire  first 
division  trigeminal  sensory  zone  (75;  296,  p.  457}.  It 
is  possible  that  collaterals  from  corneal  pain  fibers 
may  evoke  reflexes  without  awareness  of  pain  in  both 
cat  and  man,  and  that  these  come  off  at  more  rostral 
levels  than  the  obex;  or  it  may  be  that  the  specific 
anatomy  in  cats  difiers  from  man.  In  either  event 
lesions  needed  to  stop  'pain  reflexes'  in  the  cat  were 

'  The  trigeminal  nerve  enters  the  upper  pons  and  one  bundle 
of  its  fibers  descends  the  length  of  the  pons  and  medulla  into 
the  uppermost  cervical  segments,  so  the  more  rostral  the  in- 
cision the  more  toward  the  periphery  the  tract  was  being  cut. 


decidedly  diflx-rent  from  those  required  to  stop  pain  in 
man. 

Recently  Goetzl  c^/.  (too)  found  upon  stimulation 
of  the  tooth  pulp  in  unanesthetized  cats  and  rabbits  a 
rise  in  arterial  pressure  and  a  decrease  in  volume  of 
the  leg,  spleen  and  kidney,  whereas  the  reverse 
changes  in  arterial  pressure  and  organ  volume  oc- 
curred when  such  stimuli  were  delivered  to  anes- 
thetized animals.  They  concluded  from  these  ob.serva- 
tions  that  the  ability  of  the  stimulus  to  produce  a  rise 
in  arterial  pressure  depended  upon  actual  perception 
of  pain  by  the  animal.  Such  a  conclusion  has  dubious 
validity  in  \iew  of  Gerard's  erroneous  deductions  from 
the  rise  of  arterial  pressure  in  her  cats. 

Animal  experiments  in  the  spinal  cord  have  cor- 
related even  more  mi.serably  with  work  in  man. 
Cadwalder  &  Sweet  (37},  after  careful  pre-  and  post- 
operative studies  in  dogs,  found  behavior  after  antero- 
lateral cordotomy  which  they  considered  evidence  of 
incomplete  loss  of  cutaneous  pain  sensibilit\'  along 
with  severe  ataxia  of  the  hind  legs.  Their  post-mortem 
material  demonstrated  incisions  of  the  type  which  pro- 
duce total  cutaneous  analgesia  and  no  ataxia  in  man. 
They  quoted  the  work  of  six  prexious  groups  who  ob- 
tained divergent  results  from  similar  animal  work; 
three  of  the  other  groups  had  been  unable  to  demon- 
strate any  definite  cutaneous  sensory  change  in  their  an- 
imals after  anterolateral  cordotomy.  Even  in  monkeys 
Mott  (195)  found  no  evidence  of  any  loss  of  pain  sensa- 
tion after  either  unilateral  or  bilateral  division  of  the 
anterior  halves  of  the  cord.  In  the  cat  Karplus  & 
Kreidl  (138)  were  unable  to  eliminate  rostral  response 
to  painful  stimuli  applied  to  the  hind  legs  even  by 
complete  hemisections,  one  on  each  side  of  the  thoracic 
cord,  five  or  more  segments  apart.  Only  when  the 
incisions  bisecting  the  cord  were  four  segments  apart 
or  less  did  noxa  to  the  legs  fail  to  e.xcite  a  response. 
From  this  they  deduced  that  pain  is  transmitted  by 
short  chains  of  neurons  crossing  the  cord  from  side  to 
side.  The  conclusion  froin  all  these  studies  is  that  the 
bulk  of  somatic  pain-conducting  axons  in  manv 
mammals  including  monkeys  do  not  maintain  a  fixed 
position  in  the  anterolateral  columns  of  the  cord. 
Happily  from  the  standpoint  of  easy  surgical  relief  of 
pain  this  position  is  usually  the  case  in  man.  However, 
if  one  had  only  reflex  l)eha\ior  in  man  as  a  guide  one 
might  still  be  confu.sed.  Thus,  when  one  of  my  pa- 
tients, after  cordotomy,  stepped  on  an  upturned  nail, 
the  analgesic  leg  briskly  withdrew.  C^urious  as  to  why 
the  leg  jumped,  he  discovered  the  heavy  nail  in  the 
sole  of  his  foot;  he  was  consciously  aware  only  of  some 
local  tina;linsf. 


465 


Br*  •    • 


-v.r 


_■  ^■*     * 


r 


<  i 

i 


.  *■  ^ « » *i 


«« 


FIG.  I.  Naked  axons  and  terminals  in 
the  cornea  of  the  monkey,  stained  with 
methylene  blue.  .1.  Beaded  axons  ram- 
ifying in  the  basal  part  of  the  epithelium 
(X  240).  B.  Epithelium  only  which  has 
been  stripped  off  substantia  propria. 
Beaded  nerve  fibers  are  terminating  ex- 
tracellularly  in  end  beads  passing  be- 
tween cells  in  the  middle  third  of  the 
epithelium  (X  400).  [From  Zander  & 
Weddell  (313).] 


At  a  number  of  areas  in  the  human  body  pain  has 
been  said  to  be  the  only  sensation  ehcitable  in  the 
normal  state.  If  this  be  true,  it  has  seemed  especially 
reasonable  to  excite  such  areas  in  animals  and  study 
the  concomitant  nervous  behavior  on  the  assumption 
that  it  may  be  correlated  with  pain  perception. 
Appropriate  stimulus  of  the  most  intensively  studied 
such  area,  the  cornea,  has  however  been  shown  clearly 
to  evoke  other  sensations  than  pain  and  will  be  dis- 
cussed more  fully.  Even  so,  pain  is  the  dominant  and 
by  far  the  most  readily  provoked  sensation  upon 
corneal  stimulus  in  man  and,  in  general,  animal  ex- 
periments in  which  the  stimulus  used  would  surely 
bring  on  pain  in  a  normal  man  have  been  useful, 
especially  in  analysis  of  action  potentials  in  nerves. 
Indeed  Beecher(i6)  considers  that  a  more  dependable 
relationship  has  been  established  between  the  action 
of  powerful  narcotics  and  the  'experimental  pain 
threshold'  in  animals  than  in  man. 


END  ORGANS  FOR  PAIN 

Normal  Skin 

The  finding  by  Goldscheider  (loi)  of  points  on  the 
skin  particularly  .sensitive  to  painful  stimuli  and  of 
other  spots  which  could  be  stuck  painlessly  with  a 
fine  needle  has  been  followed  by  efforts  to  locate  a 


particular  end  organ,  the  nervous  receptor  for  pain. 
vonFrey's  (273,  274)  exhaustive  studies  with  calibrated 
hairs  and  thorns  led  him  to  insist  on  a  distinction  be- 
tween the  spots  in  which  the  sole  threshold  response  to 
a  point  stimulus  was  a  sense  of  pain  and  those  in  which 
it  was  a  sense  of  pressure.  Nerves  end  in  the  skin  in  a 
wide  variety  of  complex  patterns  or  specialized  end  or- 
gans (see  other  chapters  for  discussion),  but  the  over- 
whelming majority  of  the  fibers  in  the  skin  terminate 
both  in  epidermis  and  dermis  without  specialized 
groupings  of  cells  about  them,  merely  as  fine  naked 
freely  ending  axoplasmic  filaments  in  an  extracellular 
position.  They  interweave  i:)ut  do  not  fuse  with  one 
another  (288).  In  the  corneal  epithelium  such  termi- 
nals are  "disposed  in  depth  throughout  its  whole 
extent,"  as  well  as  "throughout  the  whole  extent  of 
the  .substantia  propria."  [See  Zander  &  Weddell's 
(313)  thorough  original  studies  and  analysis  of  the 
massive  literature  on  the  subject.]  Figure  i  illustrates 
the  appearances  in  two  types  of  preparation.  Weddell 
and  others  (personal  communication)  have  also  noted 
a  gross  variability  from  week  to  week  in  the  number  of 
clusters  of  corneal  naked  nerve  endings  simulating  in 
appearance  a  Krause's  end  bulb,  an  observation 
which  indicates  that  the  normal  nerve  endings  may  be 
in  a  constantly  changing  dynamic  state. 

The   multitudinous  plexiform   endings  have   been 
correlated  with  the  multiplicity  of  '  pain  points'  found 


466 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


on  examination  with  calibrated  thorns.  Strughold 
(256)  found  up  to  200  per  cm^  and  Woollard  (307), 
testing  sensation  over  a  small  area  of  his  own  thigh 
after  removal  of  each  of  a  succession  of  thin  slices  of 
skin,  found  pain  to  be  the  most  superficial  as  well  as 
the  most  extensive  in  depth  of  the  modalities  tested. 
In  one  region  of  his  own  epidermis  which  was  es- 
pecially sensitive  to  a  needle  tip  he  saw  histologically 
a  plexus  of  finely  beaded  nerve  fibers.  Woollard  et  al. 
(310)  examined  a  biopsy  of  human  skin  taken  just 
distal  to  an  ulcer  made  8  days  earlier  with  solid  carbon 
dioxide.  Pain  was  the  only  sensation  elicitable  from  the 
tissue  and  the  subsequent  microscopic  examination 
revealed  only  fine  naked  nerve  terminals.  However, 
when  Foerster  &  Boeke  (cited  in  77,  p.  16)  examined 
sections  of  skin  in  man  following  division  of  cutaneous 
nerves  and  beginning  regeneration,  they  found  in 
areas  from  which  pain  was  the  only  type  of  sensation 
no  free  intraepithelial  nerve  endings.  The  positive 
findings  of  the  previous  authors  are  probably  more 
significant.  Despite  the  density  of  the  pain  points  there 
are,  however,  spots  analgesic  to  pain.  Tindall  & 
Kunkle  (265)  find  that  these  are  still  demonstrable 
both  during  induced  erythema  (with  lowered  thresh- 
old) and  during  experimental  ischemia  (with  raised 
threshold).  They  conclude  that  the  analgesic  spot 
represents  a  gap  in  the  fiber  network  rather  than  a 
pain  fiber  with  an  unusually  high  threshold.  The 
great  ease  with  which  pain  is  elicited  from  the  cornea, 
the  tympanic  membrane  and  the  dental  pulp — all 
structures  probably  supplied  only  with  delicate  free 
filamentous  nerve  terminals  and  no  specialized  nerve 
endings — has  convinced  most  workers  that  such 
endings  can  initiate  nervous  impulses  giving  rise  to 
pain.  [For  the  neurohistology  of  the  tympanic  mem- 
brane see  Wilson  (300).] 

There  has  been  less  agreement  as  to  whether  or  not 
other  modalities  of  sensation  might  be  evoked  via 
these  delicate  nerve  endings.  Waterston  (283)  actu- 
ally believed  that  the  nerves  of  the  normal  epidermis 
mediate  only  touch,  since  he  could  slice  this  tissue 
painlessly  from  himself  with  a  razor.  In  the  skin 
shavings  he  saw  nerve  fibers  ending  in  loops  and  fine 
arborescent  figures.  In  Woollard's  (307)  detailed  de- 
scription of  his  similar  studies  he  says  that,  when  the 
first  5x22  mm  slice  of  his  skin  was  cut,  he  felt  pain 
only  at  four  previousy  mapped  'pain  spots'.  But  he 
must  have  felt  touch  during  the  rest  of  the  slicing 
process  and  this  is  likely  to  have  been  mediated,  at 
least  in  part,  by  the  other  fine  nerve  terminals  in  the 
epidermis.    Personal   repetition   of  studies  with   cali- 


brated hairs  and  needle  points  along  with  a  review  of 
much  of  the  voluminous  literature  on  cutaneous  sensa- 
tion led  us  in  1955  (296,  p.  10)  to  record  agreement 
with  Goldscheider's  (loi)  original  observation — that 
the  threshold  sensation  to  minimal  stimulation  with  a 
minute  pointed  needle  is  one  of  touch  at  the  great 
majority  of  all  spots  on  the  skin  of  the  body.  This  I 
noted  even  in  areas  specifically  recommended  by  von 
Frey  (274)  for  eliciting  pain  in  preference  to  touch, 
such  as  the  skin  over  the  eyelids,  the  biceps  brachii 
and  the  clavicle.  Consequently,  the  cutaneous  sense  of 
touch  being  even  more  widespread  than  that  of  pain, 
it  .seems  likely  that  proper  stimulus  to  many  or  even 
any  of  the  most  widespread  endings,  the  fine  un- 
myelinated type,  elicits  normally  a  sense  of  touch. 

Cornea 

Because  the  great  majority  of  anatomists  find  only 
such  fine  endings  in  the  cornea  its  sensation  has  been 
much  tested.  General  teaching  since  von  Frey  (273) 
and  in  agreement  with  Lewis  (171)  has  been  that  one 
may  evoke  only  pain  from  the  cornea.  Since  even  a 
speck  of  grit  on  the  normal  eyeball  is  so  intensely 
painful,  repeated  reports  in  the  literature  that  a  sense 
of  touch  may  be  elicited  from  the  cornea  found  little 
general  acceptance.  However,  many  normal  people 
do  in  fact  descrilje  only  a  sense  of  touch  without  pain 
or  annoyance  when  a  wisp  of  cotton  rests  on  the 
cornea. 

Lele  &  Weddell  (163)  have  recently  summarized  40 
publications  on  corneal  sensation  and  have  carried  out 
a  series  of  critical  experiments  which  may  well  become 
a  cla.ssic  of  well-controlled  study  in  the  complicated 
field  of  sensation.  In  25  of  the  40  earlier  publications 
the  various  authors  record  a  feeling  of  touch  upon 
corneal  stimulation,  and  Lele  &  Weddell  obtained 
this  response  invariably  from  each  of  10  subjects  when 
a  fine  nylon  suture  was  brought  into  contact  with  the 
cornea.  Reports  of  each  subject  never  "included  even 
a  suggestion  of  pain".  Contrariwise,  a  heavier  nylon 
thread  touching  the  cornea  caused  invariably  a  blink 
and  a  report  of  sharp  pain.  In  their  further  studies  a 
jet  of  air  at  warm,  cold  or  neutral  temperature,  a  warm 
or  cool  copper  cylinder,  or  an  infrared  beam  of  radi- 
ation was  applied  to  and  restricted  to  the  cornea.  The 
stimulus  excited  an  appropriate  sensation  of  tempera- 
ture in  the  overwhelming  majority  of  instances. 

From  such  findings  it  seems  likely  that  the  fine 
naked  nerve  endings  in  the  surface  layers  of  the  body 
are  capable  of  setting  up  impulses  which  will  enaijle  a 


467 


man  to  distinguish  not  only  a  potentially  noxious  from 
a  harmless  stimulus — pain  from  touch — but  warmth 
and  cold  as  well. 


Ahn 


il  Skin 


There  are,  however,  areas  of  abnormally  innervated 
skin  in  man  from  which  only  pain  can  be  aroused. 
Weddell  (285)  studied  biopsies  of  such  skin  from  a 
patient  with  a  lesion  of  the  sciatic  nerve  and  from  a 
patient  with  a  plastic  tube  pedicle  of  the  abdominal 
wall.  On  histologic  examination  he  saw  in  each  speci- 
men only  fine  nerve  fibers  giving  rise  to  superficial 
nerve  nets  with  beaded  endings. 

Special  Cutaneous  Sensory  Endings 

Discussion  of  the  functions  of  the  elaborate  cu- 
taneous sensory  nerve  endings  of  Meissner,  Ruflini  and 
Krause  and  of  the  deeper  such  endings,  the  Vater- 
Pacinian  corpuscles  and  neurotendinous  endings  of 
Golgi  is  germane  to  our  theme  only  to  point  out  that 
many  of  them  have  a  long  slender  'accessory  fiber' 
(Reniak  fiber  or  Timofevew  fiber)  with  a  fine  un- 
myelinated naked  nerve  ending  similar  to  those  in 
the  cornea.  Assuming  that  the  most  elaborate  forms 
of  sensory  nerve  terminal  subserve  some  specialized 
function  such  as  touch,,  warmth  or  coolness,  does  ex- 
cessive stimulation  of  such  a  receptor  cause  pain  as 
well,  and  if  so  i's  that  pain  mediated  via  impulses  in  the 
accessory  fiber?  VVoollard  (308),  in  support  of  this 
hypothesis,  has  illustrated  an  'accessory'  fiber  derived 
from  what  he  calls  the  'subepidermal  pain  plexus' 
terminating  at  a  Krause's  end  bulb.  Lavrenko  (158} 
and  K0I0S.SOV  have  shown  that  the.se  accessory 
fibers  are  not  connected  with  the  sympathetic  system; 
their  specific  association  with  pain  remains  at  the 
moment  a  speculation.  Trotter  &  Davies  (270)  re- 
garded the  sensation  of  'hot'  as  a  combination  of 
warmth  and  pain,  the  sensation  of  'cold'  likewise  would 
combine  coolness  and  pain.  With  increasing  thermal 
difference  the  sense  of  temperature  disappears  and 
pain  alone  is  perceived.  (See  above  for  comments  of 
Nafe  on  sensations  arising  from  smooth  muscle.) 
Elucidation  of  all  the  mechanisms  of  the  combined 
forms  of  sensation  is  a  task  for  the  future;  but  one  can 
say  that  the  'intensive'  theory  of  pain  is  right  to  this 
extent,  that  sufficiently  pronounced  mechanical  and 
thermal  stimulation  of  fine  unmyelinated  nerve  end- 
ings will  cau.se  pain. 


Deeper  Somatic  and  Visceral  Receptors 

Correlation  of  deeper  somatic  and  visceral  re- 
ceptors with  particular  types  of  pain  or  other  sensa- 
tion is  likewise  in  an  elementary  stage.  Free  unmye- 
linated nerve  endings  occur  in  serous  membranes,  the 
subserous  coat  of  gut,  intermuscular  connective 
tissue,  tendon  surface  and  substance,  deep  fa.scia  and 
periosteum — from  all  of  which  the  suitable  stimulus 
evokes  pain.  [For  specific  references  see  White  & 
Sweet  (296,  p.  15).]  The  plexus  of  nerve  fibers  is 
much  better  developed  in  the  adventitia  and  mu.scu- 
laris  of  arteries  than  of  veins,  according  to  Dogiel  (66). 
This  finding  correlates  well  with  the  severe  pain  com- 
monly felt  on  arterial  puncture  in  man  in  contrast  to 
the  absence  of  or  minor  pain  on  venepuncture  (283). 

Terminating  also  in  clo.se  relation  to  capillary  walls 
are  fine  unmyelinated  endings  derived  from  sheathed 
stem  fibers  of  dorsal  root  origin  described  by  Weddell 
et  al.  (288).  Their  afferent  function  is  further  .sug- 
gested by  Landis'  (151)  observation  in  man  that  pain 
occurred  when  his  micropipette  penetrated  these  tiny 
channels. 


TERMINAL  SENSORY  PLE.XUSES 

The  nerve  fibers  ramifying  in  the  subcutaneous 
tissue  and  skin  are  so  interwoven  as  to  give  the  im- 
pression of  a  continuous  net  or  syncytium,  but  even  in 
densely  innervated  areas  such  as  the  cornea  Zander  & 
Weddell  (313)  have  never  seen  fusion  between 
daughter  axons  originating  from  neighboring  nerve 
fibers,  although  they  have  occasionally  seen  nets 
formed  by  fusion  of  daughter  axons  arising  from  the 
same  parent  fiber.  Even  though  the  stem  nerve  fiber 
from  one  dorsal  root  ganglion  cell  supplies  a  large 
area  of  skin,  the  capacity  to  perceive  and  localize  pain 
correctly  to  a  single  spot  is  well  known.  It  appears  to 
be  mediated  by  the  multiple  innervation  of  each  'spot' 
by  branches  from  different  stem  fibers.  Thereby  a  tiny 
area  of  skin  gives  ri.se  to  a  pattern  of  excitation  differ- 
ing enough  from  its  neighbor  to  permit  localization 
and  two-point  discrimination.  This  disposition  of  stem 
nerve  fibers  was  first  seen  by  Bethe  (23)  at  the  sensory 
end  organs  of  frog  tongue.  Boring's  (30)  penetrating 
analysis  of  sensation  in  his  own  forearm  after  deliber- 
ate division  of  a  cutaneous  nerve  led  him  to  the  same 
concept.  Weddell  (285,  286)  was  the  first  to  demon- 
strate histologically  in  human  skin  biopsies  that  a  spot 
of  skin  especially  sensitive  to  one  modality  of  sensation 
was  in  fact  supplied   by   two  or  more  nerve  fibers 


468 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


approaching  from  different  directions.  Moreover,  in  a 
patient  with  partial  interruption  of  an  ulnar  nerve 
associated  with  impaired  sensiijilit}-,  a  shavina;  of  skin 
3  cm^  from  the  dorsum  of  the  hand  in  the  hyper- 
sensitive zone  revealed  in  one  area  a  single  well- 
stained  nerve  fiber  amid  other  unstained  and  pre- 
sumaiily  degenerated  fibers.  This  fiiaer  terminated  in  a 
net  immediately  beneath  the  epithelium,  covering  a 
roughh-  circular  area  of  0.75  cm  in  greatest  diameter. 
This  distance  corresponded  to  the  limen  of  two-point 
discrimination  for  pain  in  a  similar  normal  area.  The 
observation  suggests  that  the  appreciation  of  the  dual 
nature  of  .such  stimulus  requires  the  separation  of  the 
points  by  about  the  diameter  of  the  terminal  net  of 
each  fiber  supplying  the  zone  in  question. 


PERIPHERAL  SENSORY  NERVE  FIBERS 

Single  Fiber  Studies 

Adrian  (i,  pp.  81-go)  was  the  first  to  record  electri- 
cal impulses  from  individual  sensory  nerve  fibers  in 
animals  following  a  variety  of  peripheral  stimuli.  He 
and  Zotterman  promptly  established  that  the  spike 
potentials  from  a  single  axon  are  uniform  in  duration 
and  amplitude,  i.e.  that  the  axon's  impulse  has  an 
all-or-nothing  character.  Moreover  the>-  showed  that 
no  particular  frequency  of  the  discharge  is  character- 
istic for  pain.  Thus  a  needle  prick  evokes  a  discharge 
which  \aries  between  the  usual  limits  for  a  number 
of  types  of  stimuli  of  around  5  to  100  per  sec.  in  each 
nerve  fiber.  Adrian  pointed  this  out  as  e\idence  that 
pain  is  not  the  result  of  excessive  stimulation  of  any 
type  of  receptor;  if  it  were,  one  would  expect  a  uni- 
formly high  rate  of  discharge.  In  confirmation  of  the 
conclusion  that  high  frequenc\'  of  discharge  is  not 
necessarily  correlated  with  pain  Adrian  el  al.  (2)  re- 
ported that  puffs  of  air  at  high  frequency  directed  to 
the  skin  of  a  frog  would  produce  fiber  discharges  up 
to  300  per  sec.  Such  stimuli  did  not  seem  to  hurt  un- 
anesthetized  frogs. 

Echlin  &  Fessard  (72)  have  also  found  in  cats  that 
they  can  drive  receptors  at  frequencies  o\er  400  per 
sec.  so  as  to  record  synchronous  afferent  discharges 
from  proximal  points  on  nerves.  The  effective  stimulus, 
a  powerfully  vibrating  tuning  fork  placed  against  the 
skin  over  the  bone  of  the  til)ia  or  ankle,  would  not 
cause  pain  in  man — further  e\idence  that  high  fre- 
quency of  discharge  in  a  sensory  receptor  or  nerve 
need  not  give  rise  to  pain  in  an  afferent  pathway  not 
ordinarily  concerned  therewith. 

Adrian  however  did  note  in  animals  that  the  dis- 


charge following;  the  painful  stimulus  of  a  heavy  needle 
prick  was  prolonged  up  to  20  sec.  The  discharge  after 
a  light  needle  prick  likely  to  evoke  only  a  .sense  of 
touch  in  man  lasted  ijut  0.2  sec.  or  less  (fig.  2).  The 
initial  frequency  of  the  discharge  was  however  the 
same  with  each  type  of  stimulus.  Although  at  the  time 
of  his  writing  the  naked  terminations  of  nerves  were 
presumed  to  be  exclusively  receptors  for  pain,  it 
would,  as  he  said,  "make  for  economy  if  one  and  the 
same  nerve  fiber  could  be  used  to  signal  nonpainful 
stimulation  by  a  brief  discharge  and  painful  stimula- 
tion bv  a  much  longer  one."  He  would  account  for 
the  difference  in  sensation  by  a  breakthrough  of  the 
long  discharge  into  areas  of  the  central  nervous 
system  inaccessible  to  its  shorter  counterpart.  Such  a 
mechanism  would  not  preclude  another  apparatus  for 
touch  with  particular  receptors  and  fibers  such  as  the 
nerve  roots  around  hair  follicles.  In  addition  to  pro- 
longed discharge  the  pain  receptors  and  fibers,  as 
studied  at  the  cornea  for  example,  also  show  slow 
adaptation,  i.e.  they  continue  to  transmit  pain  im- 
pulses as  long  as  the  noxious  stimulus  is  present. 
Studies  of  single  afferent  fibers  in  the  cat  by  Maru- 
hashi  rl  al.  (185)  are  mentioned  later. 


FIG.  1.  .Action  potentials  in  cat's  cutaneous  nei'\e  in  response 
to  touch  and  pain.  Needle  on  weighed  leser  lowered  on  to  the 
skin  and  allowed  to  rest  there.  .-1.  Weight  on  needle  3  gm, 
very  brief  discharge.  B.  Weight  on  needle  43  gm,  continued 
discharge.  C  Weight  on  needle  99  gm,  continued  discharge. 
The  3  gm  weight  on  needle  used  by  Adrian  for  tracing  A  would, 
he  says,  be  on  human  skin  the  stimulus  for  the  sensation  of 
contact.  The  discharge  of  impulses  lasted  about  0.2  sec.  At 
weights  above  20  gm  a  distinct  prick'  is  felt  on  human  skin. 
Discharges  as  in  tracings  B  and  C  lasted  as  long  as  20  sec. 
[From  Adrian  (i).l 


469 


Tower  (267)  has  shown  that  stimuli  to  various  por- 
tions of  the  field  of  ramification  of  the  same  fiber 
produce  different  responses  in  that  one  fiber;  this  intro- 
duces another  variable  in  the  data  presented  to  the 
brain  increasing  the  likelihood  of  precise  spatial 
discrimination  peripherally.  She  worked  in  the  corneo- 
conjunctival  region  of  the  cat  using  a  preparation  con- 
taining but  one  to  three  fibers.  The  stimuli  with  hairs, 
needles  or  glass  rods  were  nearly  all  well  above 
threshold  and  would  presumably  have  caused  pain  at 
the  human  cornea.  She  made  oscilloscopic  recordings 
of  the  action  potentials  from  the  preparation,  noting 
that  one  isolated  nerve  fiber  yielding  fairly  large  im- 
pulses fanned  out  over  roughly  one  fourth  of  the 
cornea  and  some  of  the  adjacent  sclera.  "Low  thresh- 
old and  slow  adaptation  characterized  the  central 
region  of  the  terminal  fields  of  individual  fibers,  and 
rapid  adaptation  more  than  high  threshold,  the 
peripheral  parts."  A  strong  stimulus  near  the  center 
of  the  field  of  a  fiber  might  push  the  frequency  of  the 
response  nearly  to  the  limit  permitted  by  the  re- 
fractory properties  of  the  fiber,  namely  about  500  per 
sec.  In  general  the  frequency,  duration  and  rate  of 
adaptation  of  impulses  within  the  field  of  one  fiber 
were  determined  by  site  as  \vell  as  by  intensity  of 
stimulus.  When  many  fibers  remained  active,  the 
normal  situation  of  course,  their  fields  overlapped  in  a 
fashion  inextricable  to  the  experimenter.  But,  pre- 
sumably, the  brain  of  the  subject  uses  all  this  informa- 
tion, analyzing  signals  from  fibers  excited  minimally 
which  encircle  fibers  excited  more  vigorously  to 
achieve  better  localization.  The  frequency  of  dis- 
charge in  the  most  excited  fiber  would  still  re\eal  the 
intensity  of  the  stimulus. 

Fiber  Dicinietcrs  and  Pain  Conduction 

Gasser  (90)  and  his  collaborators  also  have  amassed 
evidence  correlating  physiological  with  anatomical 
properties  of  nerve  fibers  (see  his  Nobel  Lecture, 
1946).  Their  classification  is  based  on  the  duration 
and  form  of  the  three  components  of  the  action  po- 
tentials in  the  fibers — the  initial  negative  spike,  then 
the  negative  and  finally  the  positive  after-potentials. 
Their  'A'  fibers  embrace  all  of  the  medullated  fibers 
in  somatic  nerves  and  some  in  the  visceral  nerves  as 
well.  The  'A'  fibers  are  divisible  into  five  subgroups 
designated  in  order  of  diminishing  diameter  by  the 
letters  alpha  through  epsilon.  The  velocity  of  con- 
duction in  these  fibers  varies  directly  with  the  di- 
ameter of  the  axon,  ranging  between  go  to  115  m 
per  sec.  for  the  largest  fibers  16  to  20  ju  in  diameter 


and  around  10  m  per  sec.  in  the  smallest  myelinated 
fibers  2  to  4  M  in  diameter  (fig.  3).  They  have  called 
the  unmeduUated  fibers  in  sensory  nerves  'C  fibers; 
these  have  a  diameter  of  2  /x  or  less  and  conduct  at 
from  0.6  to  2  m  per  sec.  Each  component  of  a  'C 
fiber's  action  potential  lasts  much  longer  than  the 
corresponding  part  of  the  action  potential  of  an  A' 
fiber.  The  action  potentials  in  most  of  the  medullated 
fibers  of  visceral  nerves  difier  so  much  from  either  of 
these  that  they  have  been  placed  in  a  separate  cate- 
gory and  called  'B'  fibers.  L'sually  a  single  elevation  is 
present  with  no  \isible  negative  after  potential. 
Gasser  &  Erlanger  found  no  such  fibers  in  the  dorsal 
roots. 

The  more  recently  introduced  designations  of 
Lloyd  (i  78)  are  also  in  current  use.  His  Group  I  fibers 
from  20  to  I  2  M  are  seen  only  in  muscular  branches 
of  nerves;  Group  II  fibers  from  12  to  6  ju  are  seen  infre- 
quently in  muscular  branches  but  present  a  larae 
peak  in  cutaneous  ner\'es;  Group  III  fibers  mainly 
from  4  to  3  /i  correspond  to  'A'  delta  and  occur  in 
nerves  to  both  mu.scle  and  skin;  and  Group  IV'  are 
unmyelinated  or  G  fibers. 

Of  the  numerous  efforts  in  animals  to  correlate 
pain  with  certain  nerve  fibers,  the  early  experiment  of 
Ranson  &  Billingsley  (219)  is  still  one  of  the  more 
widely  cited.  As  the  posterior  rootlets  enter  the  spinal 
cord  they  divide  into  a  lateral  bundle  of  fine,  mostly 
unmyelinated,  fibers  and  a  medial  bundle  of  large 
fibers.  These  authors  found  that  after  .section  of  the 
small  (lateral)  fibers  stimulation  of  the  remainder  no 
longer  evoked  the  pain  reflexes'  of  struggling,  altered 
breathing  and  arterial  pressure,  whereas  after  section 
of  the  large  (medial)  fibers  these  reflexes  persisted. 
More  specifically  pain  impulses  have  been  associated 
both  with  the  delta-epsilon  'A'  fibers  and  with  the  'C' 
fibers.  The  most  direct  evidence  of  as.sociation  of  pain 
with  impulses  in  delta-epsilon  fibers  was  obtained  by 
Heinbecker  et  al.  (i  25)  from  the  cutaneous  nerves  of  a 
man's  leg  which  was  later  amputated.  No  sensation 
was  evoked  at  operative  exposure  of  the  nerves  until 
stimuli  at  a  frequency  of  1 2  per  5  sec.  caused  grimacing 
and  a  verbal  report  of  unequivocal  pain  (as  if  he  were 
being  whipped).  At  the  efl"ecti\e  stimulus  parameters 
there  was  a  clear-cut  delta  elevation  in  the  action 
potential  from  a  companion  nerve  in  the  leg,  but  no 
'C'  fiber  activity.  The  threshold  for  the  'C'  fiber  eleva- 
tion in  this  nerve  was  five  times  as  high.  Earlier 
Bishop  &  Heinbecker  (28)  had  established  in  animals 
that  the  thresholds  of  response  to  electrical  stimula- 
tion of  fibers  in  peripheral  nerve  trunks  varied  in- 
versely with  the  fiber  diameter.  So  in  their  study  in 


470  HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOG\'   ^   NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


0.5  msec.  1.0 
16-6.5  At 


1.5 


2.0 


2.5  3.0 

4.5-2.5/x 


3.5 


6.5-4.5/0. 


FIG.  3.  Action  potential  form  in  a  human  sensory  nerve.  This  curve  was  calculated  from  the  fiber 
distributions.  Inset  graph,  upper  right,  gives  fiber  distribution  for  the  medial  cutaneous  nerve  as 
fiber  diameters.  Beneath  the  curve  is  indicated  the  position  of  the  axon  potentials  according  to 
diameters  of  fibers,  not  axons.  [From  Gasser  (8g).] 


man,  since  no  pain  had  been  caused  by  weaker 
stimuli  activating  fibers  conducting  more  rapidly  and 
at  lower  threshold  than  the  delta-epsilon  group,  the 
authors  thought  these  fibers  were  specific  in  their 
pain-producing  power.  Almost  as  direct  evidence  to 
nearly  the  same  effect  has  been  secured  by  Brookhart 
et  al.  C34)  from  the  tooth  pulp  in  cats  and  man,  a 
structure  chosen  because  of  the  assumption  that  pain 
is  the  only  sensation  experienced  when  it  is  stimulated. 
These  workers  saw  in  the  cats  the  unmyelinated 
terminal  arborizations  join  to  form  a.xons  1.5  to  6  /n 
in  diameter  with  a  median  at  3  yu;  the  axons  were 
nearly  all  invested  with  a  myelin  sheath,  a  fact  already 
observed  in  human  tooth  pulp  by  Brashear  (33). 
Brookhart  et  al.  found  that  strength-duration  curves  of 
responses  upon  tooth  pulp  stimulation  in  both  cat  and 
man  were  similar  to  tho.se  obtained  for  'A'  gamma- 
delta  fibers  in  the  cat's  saphenous  nerve  and  markedly 
different  from  the  'A'  alpha  and  'C  fibers  in  the  same 


nerve.  The  index  of  excitation  in  the  cat  was  the 
action  potential  recorded  from  the  saphenous  or 
trigeminal  nerve  and  in  man  was  the  minimal  sensa- 
tion of  pain.  The  conduction  velocity  of  the  responses 
in  the  cat's  mandibular  ner\e  ranged  from  30  to  45  m 
per  sec,  putting  them  well  into  the 'A'  gamma  group. 
[The  delta  component  conducts  at  15  to  20  m  per 
sec,  according  to  Gasser  (89).] 

The  strength  of  stimulus  required  to  produce  'C 
fiber  activation  in  vivo  has  not  been  attained  in  critical 
human  study,  but  Clark  et  al.  (42)  have  shown  that 
activity  in  these  fibers  also  is  correlated  with  nocicep- 
tive reflexes.  Thus  in  deeply  anesthetized  animals  a 
stimulus  exciting  'C  plus  'A'  fibers  was  followed  by 
much  larger  reflexes  than  one  exciting  only  'A'  fibers. 
Moreover  the  'A'  fiber  conduction  was  not  necessary 
for  the  production  of  reflexes  which  could  still  be 
evoked  after  block  of  all  W  fibers  by  a  pneumatic 
cuff  surroimding  the  ner\e.  Zotterman  (315)  recorded 


47' 


only  'A'  delta  and  'C  fiber  activity  from  the  saphenous 
nerve  of  a  cat  when  the  corresponding  skin  was  burned 
by  a  special  stimulus  applied  without  mechanical  de- 
formation of  the  surface.  He  obtained  similar  records 
upon  etching  the  skin  with  acids.  Maruhashi  et  al. 
(185)  have  recently  studied  preparations  of  single 
afferent  fibers  in  the  cat  so  that  their  conclusions  as  to 
fiber  size  are  derived  from  direct  measurement.  They 
found  one  group  of  large  and  another  of  small 
'nociceptive'  fibers  in  the  range  3  to  1 1  m-  Activity  in 
such  fibers  was  evoked  by  a  pin  prick  or  strong  pull  on 
a  hair.  The  receptive  field  of  a  fiber  in  the  toe  pad  was 
2  X  2  to  3  X  3  mm;  it  was  about  10  times  larger  in  a 
hairy  area.  The  extent  of  the  field  was  clearly  defined 
and  within  it  the  receptive  spots  were  densely  dis- 
tributed. Following  the  stimuli  used  in  the.se  studies 
the  impulse  discharges  were  phasic  and  ended  in  about 
0.2  sec. ;  but  if  a  scalded  area  was  stimulated  mechani- 
cally a  protracted  after-discharge  was  present  in  both 
small  myelinated  and  unmyelinated  fibers. 

Double  Pain  Responses  or  Second  Pa/n 

From  the  foregoing  type  of  ob.servation  it  has  been 
concluded  that  pain  is  conducted  in  meduUated  'A' 
fibers  at  15  to  45  m  per  sec.  and  in  unmyelinated  'C 
fibers  at  less  than  2  m  per  sec.  The  gap  between  the 
two  groups  of  impulses  is  conceivably  sufficient  to 
permit  a  perceptible  differentiation  between  the  slow 
and  the  fast  group  and,  indeed,  long  before  the  speeds 
of  conduction  in  sensory  nerves  were  known  a  double 
pain  response  to  a  single  stimulus  was  described  by 
many  observers.  Thus  Rosenbach  (225)  in  1884  and 
Gad  &  Goldscheider  in  1892  (86)  thought  the  re- 
sponse to  a  pin  was  an  immediate  sensation  of  prick 
followed  after  an  interval  without  sensation  by  a 
second  prick.  Thunberg  (264)  investigated  what  he 
considered  to  be  two  separate  prick  sensations  with  a 
great  difference  in  reaction  time  between  the  two. 
Zotterman  in  1933  (3 14)  first  associated  the  'second' 
pain  with  'C  fiber  conduction.  He  confirmed  the 
observation  by  Lewis  el  al.  (173)  that  a  compression 
cuff  around  the  arm  blocking  the  circulation  causes 
early  paralysis  of  the  sense  of  touch,  alters  the  pain 
sense,  but  does  not  cause  analgesia  even  after  arrest 
for  40  min.  These  two  groups  of  workers  fell  in  line 
with  Ga.sser  &  Erlanger's  (91)  conclu.sion  that  arrest 
of  the  blood  flow  to  the  nerves  causes  a  progressive 
loss  of  their  function  in  accordance  with  the  character 
of  the  fiber.  The  first  fibers  blocked  are  those  in  the 
'A'  delta  elevation;  with  progression  of  asphyxia  the 
larger  medullated  'A'  fibers  are  next  affected;  finally 


after  even  the  largest  fibers  are  no  longer  conducting 
the  'C  fibers  are  blocked.  Zotterman,  using  his  com- 
pression cuff  to  switch  off  all  the  'A'  fibers,  found  that 
the  pain  which  persists  is  felt  only  after  a  delay,  and 
his  measurement  of  the  time  of  this  delay  agreed  well 
with  the  reaction  time  for  'second'  pain  recorded  by 
Thunberg.  The  conduction  velocity  in  the  sensory 
fibers  (calculated  from  the  reaction  time)  was  not  lower 
than  0.5  m  per  sec.  which  is  only  slightly  below  the 
conduction  rate  of  the  slowest  'C  fibers  in  mammalian 
nerves  observed  by  Erlanger  &  Ga.sser.  Upon  checking 
the  differences  in  time  of  appearance  of  the  second 
pain  in  relation  to  the  site  stimulated,  Lewis  & 
Pochin  (174)  found  the  expected  shorter  time  when 
thigh  rather  than  toe  was  the  area  pricked.  Thus 
calculated,  the  rate  of  conduction  in  the  limb  of  the 
second  response  was  again  at  the  'C  fiber  speed  of 
0.5  to  I  m  per  sec.  Confirmatory  evidence  of  this  con- 
cept has  arisen  from  studies  upon  cocainization  of 
nerve  fibers.  Gasser  (89)  found  this  drug  blocked  the 
'C  fibers  in  his  animals  early,  and  then  blocked  the 
medullated  A'  fibers  in  the  same  way  as  asphyxia, 
i.e.  beginning  first  with  the  smallest.  He  points  out 
that  "it  is  misleading  to  state  that  asphyxia  blocks  the 
large  fibers  first,  while  cocaine  blocks  the  small  fibers 
first."  But  cocaine  does  block  the  'C'  group  before  the 
'A'  group,  and  corresponding  with  this  Lewis  & 
Pochin  (174)  found  that  in  man  "cocaine  reduces  and 
ultimately  abolishes  the  second  pain  response,  before 
it  similarly  affects  the  first  pain  response."  They  are 
both  agreed  that  there  are  great  difficulties  with  any 
further  attempt  to  correlate  in  a  clear-cut  way  sensory 
function  and  fiber  size,  that  the  fibers  belonging  to 
different  modalities  must  be  widely  distributed 
throughout  the  various  fiber  sizes  and  that  there  seems 
to  be  little  possibility  of  associating  any  one  sensation 
with  an  elevation  in  the  electroneurogram.  Sinclair  & 
Hinshaw  (248,  249),  after  an  extensive  study  of  com- 
pression and  pressure  block  of  peripheral  nerves  in 
man,  subscribe  wholeheartedly  to  the  notion  that 
such  association  is  impossible.  Even  after  a  large 
number  of  experiments  with  procaine  they  found  it 
impossible  to  generalize  as  to  the  order  of  loss  of  the 
various  modalities  of  touch,  pain,  warmth  and  cold 
since  by  suitable  adjustment  of  the  experimental  con- 
ditions "almost  any  desired  order  of  sensory  loss  may 
be  recorded." 

Lewis  (171)  and  Gasser  (89)  are  agreed  that  both 
the  fast  and  slow  impulses  evoke  the  same  quality  of 
sensation.  Lewis  adds  that  brief  noxious  stimulation 
produces  the  sensation  of  'pricking'  and  that  a  pro- 
longed no.xious  stimulation  elicits  a  sense  of 'burning.' 


472 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Hardy  et  al.  (i  i8,p.  133)011  the  other  hand  think  there 
are  two  different  quahties  of  pain  independent  of 
the  duration  of  stimulation,  that  'pricking'  pain  is 
predominantly  "fast'  and  primarily  conveyed  by 
myelinated  fibers,  whereas  'burning'  pain  is  pre- 
dominantly 'slow'  and  conveyed  by  unmyelinated 
fibers.  Sinclair  &  Hinshaw  (248,  249)  had  made  a 
similar  statement,  but  had  added  that  "in  the  experi- 
ence of  pin  prick,  the  factor  which  determines  whether 
the  subject  reports  a  feeling  of  pain  or  not  is  probably 
not  the  initial  prick  conveyed  by  fast  fibers,  but  what 
we  may  term  'unpleasantness'  which  arrives  subse- 
quently by  the  slower  fibers."  They  also  state  that 
"after  the  slow  unpleasantness  is  removed  in  pressure 
blocks  there  is  a  period  when  it  is  exceedingly  difficult 
to  determine  whether  a  pin  prick  should  be  reported 
as  'sharp'  or  as  'sharp  and  painful'." 

Landau  &  Bishop  (150)  have  extended  the  analysis 
of  pain  sensation  by  the  techniques  of  differential 
block  by  cuff  pressure  and  procaine  to  subcutaneous 
tissue,  periosteum,  muscle  and  fascia.  They  elicited 
pain  by  both  electrical  stimulation  and  chemical  in- 
flammation. They  concluded  that  the  presumably 'C 
fiber  pain  is  "of  slower  onset,  but  of  severer  and  more 
penetrating  character  and  of  longer  after-effect." 
Periosteum,  muscle  and  veins  were  found  to  be  sup- 
plied by  fibers  of  both  types,  whereas  inflammatory 
pain  from  the  subcutaneous  injection  of  turpentine 
and  from  bee  stings,  as  well  as  the  pain  following  in- 
jection of  5  per  cent  sodium  chloride  solution,  was 
assigned  almost  entirely  to  activity  in  'C  fibers.  De- 
tailed consideration  of  their  results  reveals  some  in- 
consistencies, however;  thus  with  procaine  block  to 
these  deep  endings  which  should  block  'C  before 
delta  fibers  "only  a  partial  loss  of  deep  pain  results 
before  prick  is  blocked."  The  pricking  pain  should 
have  been  the  last  to  go  if  the  authors'  hypothesis  were 
to  be  fully  confirmed. 

Further  evidence  that  two  different  qualities  of  pain 
may  result  from  different  types  of  responses  of  the 
same  peripheral  nerve  was  obtained  by  Pattle  & 
Weddell  (206)  in  an  experiment  which  included 
direct  exposure  of  one  of  VV'eddell's  own  digital  nerves. 
The  threshold  sensation  following  single  graded  con- 
denser shocks  to  the  ner\-e  was  a  "pain  of  unpleasant 
quality  like  a  wasp  sting."  This  response  occurred  at 
all  strengths  of  shock  from  o.  i  to  6  ^f  capacity  of  con- 
denser. But  the  discharge  of  a  condenser  of  7  nf 
capacity  "produced  a  long-lasting,  severe,  aching 
pain,  which  was  completely  different  in  quality  from 
the  wasp  sting  reaction."  The  reaction  time  from 
stimulus  to  closure  of  an  electrical  contact  b\'  the  sub- 


ject was  exactly  1.27  .sec.  for  each  type  of  pain,  how- 
ever. The  study  was  carried  out  on  a  nerve  in  which 
only  a  few  fibers  appeared  to  be  responding  to  stimuli 
following  injection  of  the  local  anesthetic  procaine 
which  produced  complete  insensitivity  of  the  whole 
distal  phalanx  of  the  finger.  The  two  types  of  pain 
here  are  however  manifestly  different  from  the  experi- 
ences recorded  in  experimental  studies  on  double  pain 
from  the  intact  skin. 

An  astute  maneuver  to  measure  the  conduction 
time  for  human  pain  sen.sation  which  eliminates  the 
time  from  cerebrum  to  motor  response  has  been  de- 
scribed by  Gordon  &  Whitteridge  (106).  The  alpha 
rhythm  of  the  human  EEG  can  be  disturbed  by  un- 
expected stimulus  to  the  skin  and  in  individuals  in 
whom  this  response  was  clear-cut  these  workers 
found  that  the  time  between  painful  stimulus  and 
alpha  interruption  averaged  about  0.25  sec.  at  both 
normal  fingers  and  toes.  The  delay  was  much  greater 
when  the  iiase  of  finger  or  toe  was  compressed  for  an 
hour  or  more  by  a  cuff  occluding  its  blood  supply. 
It  averaged  1.04  sec.  from  the  asphyxiated  finger 
and  1.40  sec.  from  the  asphyxiated  toe.  In  the  latter 
state  they  measured  the  fiber  conduction  time  for 
the  'second'  pain  at  about   i   m  per  sec. 

Sinclair  &  Hinshaw  (248,  250)  and  Sinclair  (247) 
have  put  forward  some  sharp  and  cogent  criticisms 
of  much  of  this  work  on  double  pain  and  'second' 
pain,  pointing  out  that  a  delay  between  stimulus 
and  perception  of  pain  also  occurs  in  procaine  blocks, 
a  situation  in  which  the  slowly  conducting  fibers 
are  supposed  to  fail  first.  Critical  scrutiny  of  the  data 
obtained  during  the  asphyxia  caused  by  cuff  com- 
pression also  leads  to  doubts  regarding  the  original 
interpretation. 

Thus,  Lewis  &  Pochin's  (174)  average  figures  in 
two  subjects  for  appearance  of  second  pain  upon  pin 
prick  of  a  normal  finger  and  toe  and  for  appearance 
of  delayed  pain  after  a  cuff  block  were  about  the  same 
at  1.2  sec.  But  when  each  subject  is  considered 
separately,  there  is  a  statistically  significant  differ- 
ence in  both  of  them  between  the  control  and  as- 
phyxial  readings.  Moreover,  Lewis  &  Pochin  (174) 
found  after  cuff  asphyxia  that  the  latency  of  the  pain 
response,  the  reaction  time,  rises  abruptly  from  0.3 
sec.  to  be  constant  at  1.5  sec.  Sinclair  &  Hinshaw 
(250)  have  recorded  delays  much  in  excess  of  this 
figure  up  to  5  sec. — longer  than  would  be  required 
for  conduction  from  finger  to  brain  of  any  normal 
'C  fiber.  Ashby  (11)  has  likewise  pointed  out  many 
recorded  instances  of  much  longer  delays  in  tabes, 
and  I  have  seen  one  such  striking  patient  who  showed 


PAIN 


473 


a  delay  greater  than  3  sec.  upon  stimulation  of  the 
forearm.  If  the  delayed  pain  under  abnormal  con- 
ditions such  as  asphyxia  is  indeed  pure  'C  fiber  pain, 
then  the  abrupt  rise  in  latency  from  0.3  to  1.5  sec. 
described  by  Lewis  &  Pochin  is  consonant  with  the 
final  failure  of  conduction  in  'A'  delta  fibers.  How- 
ever, Wortis  el  al.  (312)  did  not  confirm  this  abrupt 
change;  they  found  delays  in  the  pain  response  at 
intervals  during  asphy.xial  compression  studies  in 
man  to  vary  upon  stimulus  to  the  foot  from  o.g  to 
1.7  sec.  Another  major  criticism  rests  upon  the  fact 
that  reaction  time  to  pain  is  influenced  greatly  by 
the  intensity  of  the  stimulus,  there  being  a  hyperbolic 
decrease  in  time  with  increasing  intensity  according 
to  Pieron  (212)  and  Eichler  (73).  In  general  the  in- 
tensity of  stimulus  has  not  been  maintained  constant 
in  the  studies  tending  to  identify  delated  pain  under 
abnormal  conditions  with  second  pain  under  normal 
conditions.  Even  the  less  complex  sensation,  touch, 
exhibits  a  reaction  time  which  \aries  in\ersely  with 
the  intensity  of  the  stimulus.  It  also  varies  with 
the  cross  sectional  area  of  the  stimulus  and  changes 
from  day  to  day,  from  subject  to  subject  and  from 
testing  site  to  testing  site  (162).  Likewise  thermal  stim- 
uli even  when  ineasured  from  the  threshold  intensity 
rather  than  from  an  absolute  zero  show  the  same 
type  of  variation  as  shown  by  Lele  &  Sinclair  (161). 
Since  the  reaction  time  represents  the  sum  total  for 
initiation  of  conduction,  for  actual  conduction  over 
both  afferent  and  efferent  paths,  for  perception  and 
judgement  and  for  synaptic  transmission,  the  assump- 
tion that  changes  in  the  reaction  tiine  are  due  to 
changes  only  in  afferent  conduction  rate  would 
seem  unwarranted. 

It  will  be  noted  that  much  of  the  work  on  second 
pain  and  double  pain  in  nerve  fibers  has  been  car- 
ried out  in  abnormal  situations  of  ischemia,  pharma- 
cologic insult  or  disease.  Weddell  et  al.  (289)  have 
suggested  that  the  delay  in  these  abnormal  conditions 
need  not  depend  on  the  existence  of  two  discrete 
groups  of  fibers  conducting  at  different  rates  and  that 
the  delay  could  occur  in  the  central  rather  than  the 
peripheral  nervous  system  as  a  consequence  of  sim- 
plification of  the  impulse  pattern  reaching  the  brain. 
Such  an  explanation  fits  more  satisfactorily  with  the 
observation  of  gross  variability  in  the  delay  and  its 
occurrence  with  all  maneuvers  depressing  conduction. 

However,  such  an  explanation  does  not  account 
for  the  occurrence  of  second  pain  under  normal  con- 
ditions. I  have  never  personally  been  able  to  con- 
vince myself  that  I  could  perceive  two  separate  pains 
in  response  to  a  single  noxious  stimulus  even  after 


following  Gasser's  (89)  prescription  of  flipping  the 
back  of  my  finger  against  a  hot  incandescent  light 
bulb  or  metal  hot  water  faucet.  Weddell  (personal 
communication)  has  had  the  .same  trouble.  And 
it  has  been  a  mystery  to  us  how  Thunberg  (264) 
and  Lewis  &  Pochin  (174)  could  measure  such  pre- 
cise reaction  times  for  a  .sensation  we  could  not  con- 
sistently discern. 

It  was  with  some  relief  that  I  read  of  Jones'  (137) 
experiments.  When  she  applied  a  rigidly  mounted 
needle  algesimeter  calibrated  in  0.25  gm  steps  to 
three  different  spots  on  the  dorsal  forearm  of  each 
of  eight  subjects,  not  one  of  them  reported  a  double 
pain  after  any  stimulus.  (The  point  of  the  needle  had 
been  sharpened  under  a  microscope  to  minimize  the 
stimulation  of  pressure  sensation.)  The  needles  were 
not  held  by  hand,  as  one  infers  was  done  b\-  previous 
investigators,  because  the  pain  stimulus  might  vary. 
In  another  effort  to  elicit  double  pain  she  permitted 
the  needle  to  remain  at  the  site  evoking  a  response. 
These  'adaptation  trials'  were  carried  out  at  the 
threshold  for  pain,  i  gm  above  threshold,  and  in 
four  highly  practiced  subjects  at  2  gm  abo\e  threshold. 
The  pain  did  not  vary  in  a  smooth  way;  instead  "the 
course  of  adaptation  showed  fluctuations;  in  about 
one-fifth  of  the  trials  there  were  only  two  peaks 
which  naive  observers  might  po.ssibly  have  inter- 
preted as  double  pain."  The  four  experienced 
subjects  looked  carefully  for  possible  double  pain, 
and  with  suprathreshold  stimuli  they  reported  it 
twice  out  of  20  trials;  in  two  other  trials  there  were 
other  types  of  double  sensation,  one  of  cold  and 
pain  and  another  of  pressure  and  pain.  The  possi- 
bility exists  that  a  suprathreshold  stimulus  may 
excite  two  discrete  receptors  sequentially  and  Jones 
suggests  this  interpretation  of  the  results.  Woollard 
et  al.  (310)  had  already  correlated  'first  pain'  with 
penetration  of  the  needle  point  into  the  epidermis 
and  "second  pain'  with  attainment  by  the  point  of 
dermal  levels.  This  they  did  by  measuring  on  the 
needle  the  depth  at  which  each  sensation  was  pro- 
voked and  then  correlating  this  with  an  actual  histo- 
logic study  of  the  skin  in  that  area. 

Thresholds  to  the  pain  upon  electrical  stimulus 
with  a  square-wa\e  pulse  from  a  Grass  stimulator 
were  also  studied  by  Jones  in  120  trials  on  each  of 
the  four  experienced  subjects.  No  double  pain  and  no 
single  delayed  pains  were  felt.  Jones  regards  this 
form  of  stimulus  as  well  suited  to  analysis  of  the  double 
pain  hypothesis  because  if  more  than  one  receptor  is 
stimulated  they  are  all  stimulated  simultaneously. 
She  points  out   that  no  experimenter  has  reported 


474 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


double  pain  with  a  single  electric  stimulus.  Heat, 
on  the  contrary,  one  of  the  more  effective  stimuli 
in  eliciting  double  pain,  does  continue  to  penetrate 
deeper  into  the  tissues  and  to  stimulate  more  remote 
receptors  even  after  the  stimulus  is  removed.  Landau 
&  Bishop  (150)  also  found  in  eight  normal,  unpreju- 
diced subjects  "only  three  who  could  recognize  a 
second  pain  response  when  the  skin  was  tested  with 
heat  or  brief  mechanical  stimuli."  They  ascribe  the 
failure  of  brief  stimuli  to  evoke  delayed  pain  to  a 
masking  effect  of  the  pricking  pain.  In  any  event  it 
would  seem  to  me  that,  since  'C  fiber  activation  in 
animals  requires  a  much  more  powerful  stimulus, 
Jones'  studies  at  threshold  levels  and  up  to  4  gm 
above  threshold  may  never  have  activated  these 
fibers. 

It  would  thus  appear  that  the  whole  suljject  of 
pain  conduction  by  nerve  fibers  of  specific  size  is 
worthy  of  careful  review.  The  technique  of  Dawson 
&  Scott  (58)  of  recording  nerve  action  potentials 
through  the  intact  skin  in  man  may  increase  the  feasi- 
bility of  securing  the  crucial  information.  The  method 
has  already  yielded  valual)ie  data  on  this  score  in 
the  hands  of  Magladery  et  al.  (183)  who  have  found 
that  the  ischemia  of  cuff  occlusion  has  a  generalized 
depressant  effect  on  the  conduction  in  both  afferent 
and  efferent  "A'  fibers  in  peripheral  nerve  trunks 
in  man.  They  studied  oscilloscopic  records  of  the 
action  potentials  and  correlated  these  with  serial 
sensorimotor  examinations  as  the  cuff  was  inflated 
and  deflated.  Thus  17  min.  after  the  onset  of  ischemia 
voluntary  power  was  still  relatively  normal.  But 
recognition  "of  all  forms  of  .sensory  stimuli  except 
those  producing  deep  pain  was  diminished."  With 
this  one  exception,  no  subject  found  one  sensory 
modality  impaired  disproportionately  to  another. 
Figure  4  taken  from  their  work  shows  the  steady  re- 
duction in  the  potential  from  the  rapidly  conducting 
fibers  as  the  ischemia  continued.  No  "C  fiber  poten- 
tials appear  in  this  record,  obtained  after  a  "maximal 
single  shock'  to  the  ulnar  nerve  in  the  low  forearm 
with  recording  over  the  ulnar  nerve  above  the  elbow. 

In  an  important  study  in  man  by  Collins,  Randt  & 
Nulsen  (unpublished  observations),  the  exposed  sural 
nerve  is  being  stimulated  distally  while  action  poten- 
tials are  recorded  oscilloscopically  from  a  more 
proximal  position.  .Such  studies  immediately  precede 
and  follow  therapeutic  incisions  into  the  pain  path- 
ways of  the  spinal  cord.  Reporting  on  the  five  pa- 
tients thus  far  studied,  they  say  tentatively  that  the 
sensation  corresponding  to  an  'A'  gamma-delta 
elevation   on   the  oscilloscope  has  been  equivocal — 


not  clear-cut  pain.  But  at  the  first  intimation  of  "C 
fiber  activation  the  patients  have  had  severe  pain; 
this  has  been  the  case  also  even  if  'A'  fiber  conduction 
was  profoundly  depressed  by  local  cooling  of  the 
nerve. 

In  relation  to  double  pain  an  effort  has  been  made 
to  generalize  even  more  widely  regarding  sense 
organs  supplied  by  nerve  fibers  of  significantly 
varying  diameters  by  Katsuki  fl  al.  (139).  They  say 
that  the  thin  fibers  carry  impulses  from  receptor 
elements  of  lower  threshold  to  physiologic  stimuli 
with  a  lower  rate  of  adaptation,  a  lower  maximal 
frequency  of  discharge  and  a  greater  tendency  to 
continuous  or  spontaneous  firing.  Bullock  (36)  has 
drawn  attention  to  the  applicability  of  this  principle 
to  nine  different  sense  organs,  the  thin  fibers  supply- 
ing the  more  sensitive  and  tonic,  the  thick  fibers  the 
more  discriminating  and  phasic  receptors.  However 
his  attempt  to  bring  pain  fibers  into  this  concept 
stands  up  only  in  the  roughest  way  under  close 
scrutiny.  He  cites  Maruhashi  ft  al.  in  his  support, 
i)ut  these  workers  actualh'  describe  distinct  tonic 
or  phasic  behavior  mainly  in  two  groups  of  small 
myelinated  nociceptive  fibers  in  the  toad  all  within 


FIG.  4.  Nerve  action  poten- 
tials in  man  following  maximal 
single  shocks  to  ulnar  nerve  in  low 
forearm.  Surface  recording  over 
ulnar  nerve  above  elbow.  Pres- 
sure cuff  on  upper  forearm  in- 
Hated  to  200  mm  Hg.  Top  record, 
before  ischemia;  lower  records,  the 
stated  number  of  minutes  after 
onset  of  ischemia.  Time:  i  and 
5  msec.  [From  Magladery  et  al 
(183).] 


475 


the  range  of  3  to  5  /x  in  diameter.  Moreover  the 
group  responding  tonically  had  a  higher  maximal 
frequency  of  discharge  than  that  responding  in  phasic 
fashion.  In  line  with  Bullock's  thought,  however, 
was  the  finding  in  both  these  groups  of  lower  ma.xi- 
mal  frequency  than  in  the  large  myelinated  nocicep- 
tive fibers  6  to  9  //  in  diameter.  These  generally  gave 
a  phasic  discharge  to  light  pin  prick,  ending  about 
0.2  sec.  after  the  onset  of  the  stimulus.  It  is  apparent 
that  useful  generalizations  from  the  welter  of  facts 
before  us  regarding  pain  and  impulses  in  nerve 
fibers    are    difficult. 


P.'MN   IN   .'>iBNOR.M.-\L  A.N.-\TO.MICAL  STATES  AT  PERIPHERY 


Dtv 


oj  Cutaneous  Nerves 


Both  the  quality  and  the  degree  of  pain  sensi- 
bility become  altered  following  injury  to  nervous 
pathways  concerned  with  its  conduction.  The  most 
painstaking  and  best  controlled  studies  of  the  changes 
have  been  made  by  investigators  who  divided  and 
then  sutured  the  cut  ends  of  one  or  more  cutaneous 
nerves  in  themselves.  They  then  followed  the  sensory 
status  during  the  period  of  recovery.  These  workers 
included  Rivers  &  Head  (223),  Trotter  &  Davies 
(270),  Boring  (30),  Sharpey-Schafer  (245)  and  Lanier 
et  al.  (157).  AH  but  the  first  group  tended  to  agree 
with  Trotter  &  Davies  that  "the  changes  consequent 
upon  depriving  a  piece  of  skin  of  its  nerve  supply 
are  distributed  in  a  central  area  of  absolute  lo.ss, 
surrounded  by  a  zone  of  much  less  loss  which  is 
slight  toward  the  periphery  and  deepens  toward  the 
center."  They  also  observed  that  the  "defect  of  sensi- 
bility to  pain  is  precisely  similar  in  character  and  dis- 
tribution to  the  defects  in  sensibility  to  cold,  to  heat 
and  to  touch."  In  addition,  Trotter  &  Davies  found 
that  there  was  an  altered  quality  to  many  sensory 
stimuli  on  the  tenth  to  twelfth  postoperative  day, 
lasting  up  to  the  sixth  or  eighth  week.  This  developed 
in  spotty,  irregular  fashion  largely  peripheral  to  the 
analgesic  zone  in  the  previously  hypalgesic  or  so- 
called  intermediate  zone.  Later  a  more  extensive 
area  of  altered  quality  of  sensation  came  on  when 
regeneration  began. 

In  the  first  'hyperalgesic'  stage  they  found  that 
pain  after  pinprick  has  an  abnormally  unpleasant 
quality,  radiates  diffusely,  tends  to  provoke  a  motor 
response,  is  poorly  localized  or  may  be  a  persistent 
severe  burning  which  may  reappear  spontaneously 
afterwards.  Two  point  discrimination  is  reduced  in 


the  area  and  touch,  although  evoked  only  by  stimuli 
normally  above  the  threshold,  may  then  have  a  pain- 
ful quality.  In  the  later  stage  of  regeneration  the  same 
qualitative  abnormalities  can  be  elicited  from  the 
previously  analgesic  zone.  These  abnormal  features 
may  persist  for  many  months  and  then  gradually 
decline.  In  general  these  reports  have  been  well  con- 
firmed but  their  significance  remains  widely  debated 
and  the  mechanisms  of  production  obscure.  The 
names  applied  to  the  situation  have  been  as  varied  as 
the  hypotheses;  'hyperpathia',  'intensification",  'dyes- 
thesia',  'over-reaction'  and  'paradoxical  pain'  have 
been  used.  Hyperalgesia  is  probably  the  least  appro- 
priate term  since  it  implies  a  lowered  threshold  to 
pain  which  is  in  fact  usually  not  the  case  in  this 
condition. 

The  development  of  the  early  phase  of  hyperpathia 
was  correlated  by  Pollock  (214)  with  the  ingrowth 
of  fibers  from  the  adjoining  peripheral  nerves  for  two 
reasons:  a)  the  early  hyperpathia  (and  the  other  re- 
covery of  sensation)  appears  long  before  regenerating 
fibers  could  reach  the  skin;  and  b)  such  .sensation 
is  not  lost  if  the  regenerating  nerve  trunk  is  cut  a 
second  time.  Weddell  and  as.sociates  (285)  have  in  fact 
demonstrated  unmyelinated  fibers  growing  out  from 
the  intermediate  into  the  originally  anesthetic  zone 
using  methylene  blue  stain  in  man.  More  recently 
Weddell  and  associates  (personal  communication) 
have  acquired  evidence  that  a  denervated  sector  of 
the  cornea  is  reinnervated  from  three  sources  of  nerve 
supply. 

Head  &  Sherren  (122)  hypothecated  that  the  nor- 
mal sensations  were  mediated  by  'epicritic'  groups  of 
nerves  and  that  the  abnormal  qualities  ensued  only 
when  'protopathic'  fibers  were  excited.  Their  com- 
plex formulation  completely  failed  to  fit  the  facts 
brought  out  h\  each  of  the  succeeding  workers  who 
studied  their  own  sensations  before  and  after  de- 
liberate cutaneous  nerve  section.  Cobb  (46)  in  his 
work  on  patients  with  peripheral  nerve  injuries  after 
World  War  I  drew  attention  to  the  fact  that  Head's 
alleged  areas  of  dissociation  of  sensibilities  arose 
from  comparing  stimuli  which  were  not  quantita- 
tively equivalent.  He  found,  for  example,  that  the 
areas  of  sensory  loss  were  coextensive  if  one  used  a 
soft  brush  to  test  cutaneous  touch  and  a  needle  point 
at  15  gm  pressure  for  pain.  .Sufiice  it  to  sav  that 
despite  the  cogency  of  the  criticisms  of  all  of  these 
workers  it  has  required  the  devastating  verbal  scythe 
of  Waishe  (282),  giving  incisiveness  to  his  keen 
critical  powers,  to  .sweep  from  the  literature  favorable 
reference  to  the  'protopathic'  and  'epicritic'  nervous 


476 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


systems  of  Head  and  associates.  We  are  still  left  with 
the  necessityof  explaining  the  above  abnormal  features 
of  the  sensory  response.  Boring  (30,  p.  92)  agrees 
with  Head  to  this  extent:  the  abnormal  sen.sory  in- 
tensity is  achieved  by  the  removal  of  an  inhibition. 
More  recently  Landau  &  Bishop  have  identified  the 
type  of  pain  which  they  consider  to  be  tran.'Oiitted 
over  normal  'C  fibers  with  the  'protopathic'  forms  of 
sensation  of  Head  et  al.  (121)  and  conclude  that 
block  of  delta-pain  fiber  responses  releases  "the  per- 
ception of  'C  fiber  pain  in  the  otherwise  normal 
subject." 

An  alternative  explanation  has  been  put  forward 
by  Weddell  et  al.  (289)  on  the  basis  of  their  histologic 
studies  of  human  skin  at  39  sites  from  which  the 
above  abnormal  qualities  of  pain  were  induced. 
Control  studies  were  made  at  20  other  sites  in  cutane- 
ous scars  from  which  the  pain  did  not  have  the  ab- 
normal unpleasant  quality.  In  the  former  group  the 
nerve  nets  and  terminals  were  isolated  from  their 
neighbors  instead  of  interweaving  with  them  as 
occurs  normally,  and  as  was  seen  in  those  reinner- 
vated  scars  which  showed  no  abnormal  quality  in 
the  senations  aroused  from  them.  Normally,  as  Bor- 
ing (30,  p.  95)  first  deduced,  "single  sensory  spots  are 
innervated  by  more  than  one  nerve  fiber  and  the 
multiple  innervation  is  projected  upon  the  central 
nervous  system  as  multiple  excitations."  Weddell 
(285)  demonstrated  this,  correlating  neurophysio- 
logic  with  clinical  findings,  and  Weddell  >i  al.  (289) 
conclude  that  the  complex  pattern  of  impulses  arising 
from  such  multiple  innervation  is  essential  to  the 
normal  quality  of  pain  "of  everyday  experience," 
whereas  if  only  a  single  pain  fiber  or  terminal  is  ex- 
cited then  the  pain  is  of  characteristic  unpleasant 
quality.  This  conclusion,  that  reduction  in  the  den- 
sity of  innervation  of  an  area  will  cause  alteration  in 
the  quality  of  pain,  has  likewise  been  reached  inde- 
pendently by  Livingston  (177).  In  a  patient  recover- 
ing from  an  injury  to  the  median  nerve  at  the  wrist 
there  was  one  area  in  which  pain  was  of  normal  t>'pe 
until  one  of  the  main  nerve  trunks  was  blocked  with 
procaine,  whereupon  pinprick  became  peculiarly 
unpleasant.  This  state  was  exacerbated  when  two 
of  the  three  nerves  were  blocked  simultaneously. 
Weddell  et  al.  (289)  have  also  studied  the  alterations 
in  pain  sensibility  in  themselves  upon  compression  of 
the  upper  limb  with  a  sphygmomanometer  cuff. 
They  found  the  first  change  to  appear  was  a  rela- 
tively abrupt  alteration  in  the  quality  of  the  pain 
upon  a  needle  prick;  when  fully  de\eloped  at  ai^out 
the  thirtieth  minute  of  compression  the  prick  caused  a 


"singularly  unpleasant  sensory  experience,"  a  slow 
swelling  burning  sting  lasting  for  as  long  as  10  sec. 
and  giving  ri.se  to  a  withdrawal  reflex  difficult  to 
control.  There  was  also  an  increasing  interval  be- 
tween the  application  of  the  stimulus  and  its  per- 
ception. They  concluded  that  these  typical  features 
of  'unpleasant  pain'  were  to  be  correlated  with  a 
gradual  reduction  in  the  number  of  fibers  conducting 
impulses   as   the   compression   continued. 

Isolation  of  the  pain  nets  appeared  to  have  no 
eflfect  on  the  threshold  of  pain  sensibility.  In  a  num- 
ber of  biopsies  these  workers  saw  abnormal  appear- 
ances of  the  nerve  endings,  ellipsoidal  expansions 
which  they  called  growth  cones.  The  lowest  thresholds 
to  pain  occurred  in  their  patients  in  whom  such 
growth  cones  lay  just  beneath  the  basal  layer  of 
epidermis.  In  such  cases  the  mere  passing  of  a  camel's 
hair  brush  across  the  area  was  painful. 

Foerster  had  earlier  (77)  .suggested  that  the  peculiar 
abnormalities  of  sensation  (his  hyperpathia)  arose 
from  the  stimulation  of  an  isolated  'pain  point.' 
The  .similarity  between  this  explanation  and  that  of 
Weddell  et  al.  is  only  superficial,  however,  because 
Foerster  contended  that  as  soon  as  other  senory 
modalities  such  as  touch  and  pressure  were  felt  the 
hyperpathia  began  to  recede,  a  viewpoint  which  is 
.simply  not  substantiated  by  the  observations  of 
others.  The  sites  at  which  pain  has  the  abnormal 
quality  are  not  directly  correlated  with  anesthesia  to 
touch.  Figure  5  shows  the  results  of  an  examination 
in  which  more  spots  hypersensitive  to  pinprick  were 
in  fact  found  outside  of  the  anesthetic  zone.  Simi- 
larly Lanier  et  al.  (157)  in  their  study  found  an  area 
in  which,  although  touch  sensibility  was  perfect,  pain 
sensibility  was  diminished  and  unpleasant  pain 
could   be   elicited   from    this   region. 

Figure  5  also  illustrates  another  observation  of 
Trotter  &  Davies  (270,  p.  170),  namely  that  such 
patterns  of  'hyperalgesia,'  as  they  called  it,  were 
associated  with  veins.  They  say  that  often  the  skin 
over  the  vein  itself  was  the  most  sensitixe  part  of  the 
patch.  Although  Cobb  also  thought  that  "these 
painful  spots  were  usualh'  along  the  course  of  a  super- 
ficial vein,"  the  explanation  of  this  is  unclear.  Trotter 
&  Davies  originally  regarded  the  hyperalgesia  as  a 
"secondary  process  due  to  the  presence  of  some 
irritating  substance  produced  as  the  result  of  the  divi- 
sion  and   degeneration   of  the  nerve." 

But,  in  the  light  of  another  17  years  of  rumination 
on  the  subject.  Trotter  (269)  thought  that  the  lack 
of  complete  insulation  of  regenerating  nerve  fibers 
would  explain  the  raised  threshold  to  stimuli  and  the 


477 


^  \ 

\\ 

r-  -^, 

FIG.  5.  Hyperpathia  in  relation  to  anesthesia  following  section  of  a  cutaneous  nerve.  Examina 
tion  34  days  after  division  of  middle  cutaneous  ner\e  of  thigh.  VVttkin  Ike  continuous  line:  anesthesia 
to  camel's  hair  brush.  Within  the  dotted  lines:  two  areas  abnormally  sensitive  to  pin.  Broken  lines  show 
course  of  superficial  \-eins;  spots  marked  X  show  maximal  sensitisity  to  pin  prick.  [From  Trotter  & 
Davies  (270).] 


exaggerated  explosive  type  of  response  upon  effectual 
stimulation.  Accompanying  this  concept  was  his 
notion  that  many  types  of  fiber  in  their  poorly  in- 
sulated regenerating  phase  may  conduct  impulses 
giving  rise  to  pain,  since  he  presumed  that  pain  was 
the  only  sensation  evoked  from  the  uninsulated,  i.e. 
naked,  unmyelinated  end  organs.  He  goes  on  to  say, 
"With  the  advance  of  regeneration  the  fibers  serving 
touch,  heat  and  cold,  become  once  more  connected 
with  end-organs,  and  then  their  insulation,  by  the 
junction  of  the  neurilemma  with  the  capsule  of  the 
end-organ,  can  be  completed.  The  completely  insu- 
lated fiber,  having  lost  its  teinporary  resemblance  to 
the  pain  fiber,  becomes  once  more  sensitive  to  the 
finer  stimuli  and  ceases  to  yield  exaggerated  re- 
sponses." Later  work  already  discus.sed  which  demon- 
strates the  capacity  of  naked  unmyelinated  endings 
to  transmit  impulses  concerned  with  touch  cuts  the 
ground  from  under  the  latter  part  of  this  reasoning, 
but  the  concept  of  lack  of  insulation  has  become  more 


appealing  since  the  work  of  Granit  et  a!.  (109). 
These  investigators  showed  that  a  generalized  break- 
down in  'insulation'  occurs  at  the  site  of  injury  to  the 
sciatic  nerve  in  cats.  This  is  so  striking  when  the  nerve 
is  cut  across  that  impulses  set  up  in  spinal  ventral 
(motor)  rootlets  are  transmitted  to  the  sensory  fibers 
at  the  cut  and  can  be  picked  up  via  an  oscilloscope 
from  the  dorsal  (sensory)  rootlets  of  the  same  seg- 
ment. Such  an  'artificial  synapse'  or  'fiber  interac- 
tion' also  develops  at  the  crush  of  a  knot  tightened 
around  a  nerve  and  even  appears  from  time  to  time 
after  moderate  pressures  of  50  to  iio  gm  against  a 
nerve  insufficient  to  stop  conduction  beyond  the 
point  of  compression.  Granit  et  al.  have  assumed 
that  pain  fibers  would  be  among  the  most  easily 
excited  by  fiber  interaction.  These  observations 
clearly  present  a  mechanism  whereby  impulses  trav- 
ersing one  fiber  may  abnormally  pass  to  many  at  a 
site  of  injury,  thereby  pennitting  abnormal  central 
excitation. 


478 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Hyperalgesic  State  After  Trauma 

We  may  now  consider  the  special  studies  of  Lewis 
(171)  on  the  hyperalgesic  state  provoked  in  skin  by 
controlled  scratching,  heat,  freezing,  ultraviolet 
light,  or  chemical  or  electrical  irritation.  These 
varied  forms  of  trauma  all  produce  a  skin  which  is 
hyperalgesic  in  the  strict  sense  of  the  word,  i.e.  a 
lighter  needle  prick  will  cause  pain  from  it  than  from 
corresponding  normal  skin.  In  addition,  an  effective 
prick  gives  unusually  intense,  diffuse  and  long-lasting 
pain.  Spontaneous  pain  is  present  which  is  worsened 
by  relatively  small  amounts  of  warmth,  more  marked 
cooling  or  light  contacts  such  as  those  from  clothing. 
The  zone  of  hyperalgesia  gradually  spreads  after  the 
injury — for  example  five  minutes  of  faradization  of 
the  skin  a  little  above  the  wrist  provoked  the  above 
change  over  an  area  18  cm  long  with  a  maximal 
width  of  7  cm,  an  extent  achieved  within  1 1  min. 
(171,  p.  69).  The  soreness  in  the  area  lasted  several 
hours.  Some  days  later  repetition  of  the  stimulation 
procedure  led  to  nearly  the  same  pattern  of  hyper- 
algesic zone.  In  general,  such  zones  tend  to  corre- 
spond to  the  entire  area  of  a  cutaneous  nerve,  as 
shown  by  their  agreement  with  the  area  of  sensory 
elimination  after  anesthetic  block  of  the  nerve  trunk. 
Moreover,  direct  stimulation  of  the  cutaneous  nerve 
trunk  or  any  of  its  smaller  branches  will  evoke  the 
typical  hyperalgesic  pattern.  Lewis  advanced  the 
view  that  a  pain-producing  substance  developed  at 
the  site  of  injury  which,  by  stimulus  to  local  nerves, 
provoked  the  reaction  over  the  whole  arborization 
of  a  single  cutaneous  nerve.  He  also  hypothecated 
that  such  reactions  must  be  mediated  by  a  new  and 
distinct  system  of  fibers  which  he  called  'nocifensor' 
nerves.  He  was  especially  led  to  this  conclusion  by  the 
fact  that  the  pain  terminals  arising  from  a  single 
nerve  fiber  have  never  been  demonstrated  histo- 
logically to  cover  so  large  an  area  as,  e.g.  7x18 
cm  on  the  forearm.  Maruhashi  et  al.  (185)  have  come 
close  to  this  electrophysiologically  however.  They 
found  individual  afTerent  fibers  in  the  cat  innervating 
oval  areas  ranging  from  3x5  to  5x9  cm.  Such 
fibers  to  which  they  give  the  special  designation  'wide 
receptive  fibers'  were  abundant  in  all  skin  nerves 
examined;  they  ranged  from  2  to  5  /j  in  diameter. 
These  fibers  are  probaljly  not  identifiable  with  the 
nocifensor  system  since  afferent  impulses  can  be 
evoked  in  them  by  'extremely  light  touch'  to  the 
skin  or  to  a  hair  in  the  fiber's  large  receptive  field. 
Moreover  such  responses  persist  in  areas  'deaffer- 
ented'  by  excision  of  five  lumbar  root  ganglia  3  to 


4  wk.  before  the  experiment.  On  the  contrary  the 
post-traumatic  hyperalgesia  of  Lewis  disappears 
from  zones  denervated  by  posterior  rhizotomy.  In 
the  absence  of  any  anatomical  demonstration  of  an 
entirely  different  system  of  fibers  specialized  to  medi- 
ate the  hyperalgesic  spread,  this  concept  of  nocifensor 
fibers  has  won  little  support.  Walshe  (282)  and 
White  &  Sweet  (296,  p.  96)  may  be  consulted  for 
further  arguments /)/o  but  mainly  con. 


CHEMIC.'\L  EXCIT.XNTS  OF  P.MN 

The  concept  that  chemicals  liberated  at  the  site 
of  injury  provoke  pain  has  been  supported  experi- 
mentally by  Lewis  (171,  pp.  113  to  115).  His  extracts 
of  freshly  excised  human  skin  caused  pain  when  in- 
jected in  tiny  quantities  intradermally.  He  thought 
the  substance  was  not  histamine  since  he  found 
this  to  give  itching  rather  than  pain  when  it  was 
pricked  into  the  skin  even  in  such  high  concentra- 
tion as  1:30.  Rosenthal  (226),  however,  found  that 
"as  little  as  fifty-four  molecules  of  histamine"  in- 
jected intradermally  will  cause  pain,  and  in  a  series 
of  papers  with  several  collaborators  has  presented 
evidence  that  histamine  or  a  similar  substance  is  the 
chemical  mediator  for  cutaneous  pain.  Moreover 
Habgood's  (113)  analyses  of  the  substance  liberated 
upon  antidromic  stimulation  of  frog  cutaneous  nerve 
pointed  toward  histamine  or  an  'H-substance.'  In 
addition  he  demonstrated  that  the  chemical  so 
produced  could  often  evoke  spontaneous  discharge 
from  an  adjacent  nerve  twig  (fig.  6).  That  a  similar 
phenomenon  may  take  place  in  man  is  intimated  by 
Foerster's  observations  upon  stimulation  of  distal 
ends  of  divided  posterior  roots  at  operation.  This 
provoked  burning  pain  in  the  skin  which  was  elimi- 
nated by  division  of  adjoining  posterior  roots.  If 
then  antidromically-induced  liberation  of  a  chem- 
ical which  stimulates  nerve  endings  will  activate  a 
separate  but  overlapping  sensory  unit,  a  wide  area 
would  be  involved  by  a  continuation  of  this  process. 
The  extent  of  the  spread  would  tend  to  increase  with 
the  extent  and  .severity  of  the  original  injur\-.  .^nd 
since  Lewis  had  already  obtained  hyperalgesia  by 
antidromic  stimulation  of  cutaneous  nerves  in  man, 
its  explanation  would  not  require  either  a  separate 
system  of  nocifensor  nerves  or  the  wide  receptive 
fibers  of  Maruhashi  et  al.  (185). 

The  technique  of  applying  fluids  to  the  exposed 
base  of  a  blister  caused  by  cantharidin  or  heat,  de- 
veloped   by  Keele   and  his   a.ssociates,   has  enabled 


PAIN 


479 


Skin 


T 

Stimulating  C 

electrodes  (^ 


Recording 
IQ  electrodes 


I 


^^j^  L^^  1^^^  ^|u^  k 


FIG.  6.  Double  nerve  preparation  from  frog's  skin  show- 
ing chemical  transmission  of  impulse  from  one  nerve  to  the 
other.  Antidromic  stimulation  of  the  peripheral  end  of  one 
cutaneous  nerve  was  often  evoked  by  electrical  stimulation 
of  an  adjoining  cutaneous  nerve  with  an  overlapping  field, 
the  responses  appearing  either  during  or  after  the  antidromic 
stimulation.  The  nature  of  the  discharges  is  shown  in  the  pho- 
tograph of  an  action  potential.  Vertical  while  lines  are  synchro- 
nous with  shock  aitefact  and  are  go  msec,  apart;  a  succession 
of  two  fast  spikes  appears  3  to  4  msec,  later ;  then  one  fast,  four 
slow,  one  fast  and  two  slow  impulses  regularly  succeed  in  the 
recording  electrodes  following  each  stimulus  to  the  other  nerve. 
Direct  conduction  from  one  nerve  trunk  to  the  other  and 
spread  of  current  could  be  excluded.  [From  Habgood  (i  13).] 


them  to  study  effectively  the  pain  produced  by 
chemical.s.  The  method  has  given  them  more  con- 
sistent results  than  intradermal  injection  or  pricking 
of  the  skin  through  a  drop  of  solution  on  it.  They  have 
demonstrated  that  tiny  amounts  of  three  identified 
substances  found  in  tissues  provoke  pain  when  applied 
to  the  area  exposed  after  removal  of  blistered  skin. 
Both  acetylcholine  and  histamine  in  concentrations 
of  io~^  gm  per  ml,  and  5-liydro.\ytryptaminc  (sero- 
tonin) even  in  amounts  as  low  as  io~*  gm  per  ml 
cause  pain.  The  time  of  onset  and  duration  of  the 
pain  are  different  and  characteristic  for  each  of  the 
three.  The  two  latter  substances  have  already  been 
associated  with  injured  tissue,  serotonin  with  platelet 
breakdown.  Saline  extracts  of  rat  and  human  skin 
made  by  these  workers  were  found  to  contain  5  x  io~^ 
gm  per  ml  of  histamine  and  to  cause  prolonged  pain 
when  applied  to  a  blister  base.  Histamine  alone  in 
such  concentration  caused   only  itching. 

The  Keele  group  has  found  an  additional  pain- 
producing  substance  (PPS)  in  the  blister  fluid  itself 
as  well  as  in  human  plasma,  serum  and  protein-rich 
inflammatory    fluids    obtained    from    pleural,    peri- 


toneal, joint  or  hydrocoele  cavities  (10).  The  PPS 
shows  the  peculiar  behavior  of  appearing  in  these 
fluids  only  after  they  are  'activated'  by  contact  with 
glass;  but  following  activation  the  capacity  to  evoke 
pain  declines  rapidly  to  less  than  10  percent  of  the 
peak  within  an  hour  at  room  temperature.  The  pain- 
producing  activity  of  PPS  correlates  well  with  ability 
to  cause  contraction  of  the  isolated  rat  uterus.  By 
applications  of  this  convenient  method  as  well  as 
by  other  tests  they  showed  PPS  to  be  different  from 
serotonin  and  histamine  and  to  resemble  the  poly- 
peptide Ijradykinin  more  closely  than  any  other  sub- 
stance yet  tested.  Bovine  bradykinin  produced  pain 
in  man  indistinguishable  from  that  of  PPS  in  concen- 
trations as  low  as  io~^  gm  per  ml.  A  continuation 
of  this  type  of  study  may  lead  to  knowledge  of  the 
actual  substances  which  are  stimulating  pain  endings 
in  vivo.  In  general  the  minutiae  of  the  mechanisms 
whereby  stimuli  are  transduced  into  nervous  impulses 
remain  wide  open  for  investigation. 


POSTERIOR  AND  ANTERIOR  ROOTS 

The  distribution  of  the  nerve  fibers  transmitting 
pain  via  each  posterior  root  has  been  largely  worked 
out  for  the  cutaneous  supply.  Various  diagrams  of 
these  so-called  dermatomes  obtained  by  a  number 
of  methods  have  been  collected  by  White  &  Sweet 
(296,  pp.  26  to  30);  such  data  are  more  fragmentary 
for  the  deeper  structures  (296,  pp.  22  and  23).  The 
possibility  that  afferent  fibers  may  also  enter  the 
cord  by  way  of  the  anterior  roots  has  continued  to 
be  supported  by  bits  of  evidence  over  the  past  75 
years.  White  &  Sweet  (296,  pp.  31  to  36)  have  col- 
lected and  analyzed  their  own  and  others'  data  on 
this  subject.  Studies  not  mentioned  in  that  account 
include  those  of  Maruhashi  el  al.  (185),  who  measured 
the  .size  of  small  myelinated  fibers  they  found  asso- 
ciated with  ganglion  cells  on  the  course  of  ventral 
roots  in  cats.  These  practically  coincided  with  their 
afferent  fibers  "with  wide  receptive  field"  (discussed 
above),  and  they  regard  it  as  "certain  that  these 
fibers  provide  an  exception  to  the  Bell-Magendie 
law."  However,  these  fibers  were  stimulated  by 
touch  rather  than  noxious  maneuvers.  We  were  un- 
able to  find  a)  record  of  any  patient  in  whom  an- 
terior rhizotomy  stopped  pain  unassociated  with 
muscle  spasm,  i)  report  of  altered  response  to  ob- 
jective sensory  tests  after  anterior  rhizotomy  or  c) 
written  account  of  failure  to  stop  pain  by  posterior 
rhizotomv  followed  by  success  af*er  a  later  anterior 


48o 


HANDBOOK  OF  PHYSIOLGG"!' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGV 


rhizotomy.  We  are  hence  without  positive  evidence 
that  pain  impulses  in  clinically  significant  numbers 
transverse  the  anterior  roots  to  enter  the  spinal  cord  in 
man. 


P.MN  .AND  .AUTONOMIC  NERVOUS  SYSrE.VI 

This  system  of  nerves  was  defined  by  Langley  (155) 
as  efferent  and  distinguished  from  the  somatic  ner- 
vous system  by  a  peripheral  synapse.  He  was  will- 
ing to  regard  as  autonomic  afferent  fibers  only  "those 
which  give  rise  to  reflexes  in  autonomic  tissues  and 
which  are  incapable  of  directly  giving  rise  to  sensa- 
tion." He  considered  all  other  afferent  fibers  somatic. 
We  have  since  his  time  become  progressively  more 
aware  that  most  of  the  nerves  of  gross  anatomy  named 
"autonomic'  contain  pain  fibers.  This  is  true  in  vir- 
tually all  of  the  sympathetic  and  pelvic  parasympa- 
thetic nerves  to  the  torso;  the  distribution  of  these 
insofar  as  they  are  known  has  ijeen  diagrammatized 
by  White  [see  figs.  127,  130,  131,  133  and  134  in 
White  &  Sweet  (296)].  Evidence  for  the  presence  of 
pain  fibers  in  these  nerves  has  been  gleaned  both 
upon  stimulation  and  after  denervation.  These  vis- 
ceral pain  fibers  differ  from  autonomic  efferents  in 
their  probable  nonstop  course  through  the  para- 
vertebral ganglia  to  enter  the  posterior  roots  of  the 
spinal    nerves. 

Sympathetic  Nerves 

The  concept  that  the  efferent  sympathetic  path- 
ways leave  the  cord  only  from  the  lowermost  cervical 
to  the  upper  lumbar  segments  and  enter  the  sym- 
pathetic chain  via  the  white  rami  communicantes  is 
so  well-established  that  it  has  led  to  ready  acceptance 
of  Langley's  statements  that  the  afferent  fibers 
travelling  with  the  sympathetic  nerves  also  return 
impulses  to  the  spinal  cord  only  by  way  of  these  white 
rami.  Thus  in  one  study  (156)  leading  toward  this 
conclusion  he  deduced  that  sensory  fibers  of  the  cat's 
major  accelerator  nerve,  the  chief  sympathetic  nerve 
to  the  heart,  entered  the  cord  only  by  the  top  5  white 
rami.  He  found  in  general  that  stimulation  of  the 
central  ends  of  the  gray  rami  produced  "no  observable 
physiological  effect"  in  cats  (155).  Largely  by  histo- 
logic study  of  degenerating  myelin  after  section  of 
various  nerves  he  concluded  that  the  cell  bodies  of  the 
afferent  fibers  travelling  with  the  sympathetic  nerves 
are  only  in  the  posterior  root  ganglia  of  the  nerves 
with  white  rami.  For  example,  upon  section  of  the 


cervical  sympathetic  trunk  below  the  superior  cer- 
vical ganglion  he  found  complete  degeneration  of 
the  fibers  rostral  to  the  cut  and  extending  up  to  the 
ganglion  C'SS)-  This  conclusion  unfortunately  rested 
on  the  misconception  that  there  are  no  nonmedullated 
afferent  fibers  (i  54)  which  we  now  know  to  be  wrong. 
His  histologic  studies  would  have  failed  to  demon- 
strate degeneration  in  these.  Ranson  (218)  reached 
the  same  conclusions,  again  relying  heavily  on  micro- 
scopic studies  of  myelin  degeneration.  He  said, 
"Histologically  it  is  possible  to  trace  sensory  fibers 
through  the  sympathetic  system  because  of  their  rela- 
tively large  size."  These  statements  were  all  made 
before  the  'C  fiber  days  of  Gasser  and  Erlanger, 
and  the  conclusions  reached  from  them  require  modi- 
fication not  only  because  of  that  work  but  also  be- 
cause of  later  observations  in  man. 

To  begin  with  the  neck,  we  point  first  to  Leriche 
&  Fontaine  (168)  who  studied  pain  in  10  operations 
on  nine  patients  by  faradic  stimulation  to  the  superior 
cervical  ganglion  and  to  the  rami  communicantes 
of  the  second  and  third  cervical  nerves.  The  pain 
was  referred  mainly  behind  the  ear  and  to  all  the 
teeth  in  the  lower  jaw.  Stimulus  to  the  trunk  just 
below  the  ganglion  caused  pain  of  similar  locus  which 
was  often  very  intense  and  might  even  last  several 
days.  Leriche  (167)  explained  this  on  the  basis  of 
stimulation  of  vasomotor  fibers  in  the  area  of  pain 
rather  than  ascribing  it  to  direct  stimulation  of  af- 
ferent fibers  in  the  sympathetic  nerves.  Foerster  et  al. 
(79,  p.  147)  likewise  produced  pain  upon  stimula- 
tion of  the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk  at  every 
operation  thereon  undertaken  under  local  anesthe- 
sia. The  pain  was  referred  somewhere  to  the  ipsilat- 
eral  side  of  the  neck  or  head.  But  they  added  the  ob- 
servation that  stimulus  to  the  caudal  cut  end  of  the 
cervical  sympathetic  trunk  likewise  caused  pain  of 
the  same  severity  and  distribution.  From  this  they 
concluded  that  the  stimulus  was  indeed  to  afferent 
fibers  directly  and  that  these  were  entering  the  spinal 
cord  lower  down.  Frazier's  (82)  results  on  stimulation 
both  to  carotid  vascular  plexuses  and  superior 
cervical  ganglia  were  less  consistent,  but  in  three  of 
four  patients  upon  stimulation  at  some  point  in  the 
above  zones  pain  was  described  in  the  ipsilateral 
head  or  neck.  Peet  (209)  has  also  produced  pain  in 
the  trigeminal  zone  "in  a  number  of  patients"  upon 
electrical  stimulation  of  the  superior  cervical  ganglion. 

To  determine  the  mechanism  of  this  pain  Davis 
&  Pollock  (55)  carried  out  a  series  of  experiments  in 
cats.  They  found  no  evidence  of  pain  on  stimulation 
of  the  intact  cervical  sympathetic  trunk,  and  Langley 


PAIN 


481 


(152)  and  Cleveland  (45)  found  none  on  stimulation 
of  the  caudal  end  of  this  trunk  after  it  had  been  cut 
below  the  superior  cervical  ganglion.  These  represent 
another  instance  in  which  studies  in  man  yield  results 
relative  to  pain  opposite  to  conclusions  drawn  from 
work  in  animals.  However,  the  cats  of  Davis  & 
Pollock  behaved  as  though  in  pain  upon  stimulation 
of  the  superior  cervical  ganglion  rather  than  of  the 
trunk  below  it  and  they  continued  to  do  .so  after:  a) 
all  its  branches  were  cut  except  those  to  the  carotid 
plexus,  b)  the  posterior  roots  of  the  upper  12  spinal 
nerves  were  cut  and  r)  the  trigeminal  posterior  root 
and  the  upper  1 1  spinal  anterior  roots  were  cut. 
Only  when /)  was  combined  with  trigeminal  posterior 
rhizotomy  were  the  pain  responses  stopped.  Since 
Davis  &  Pollock  accepted  the  evidence  of  Langley 
and  Ranson  that  there  are  no  afferent  pathways  in 
the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk,  they  explained  their 
findings  on  the  basis  that  they  were  setting  up  efferent 
sympathetic  impulses.  These  were  presumed  to  pro- 
duce a  peripheral  effect  which  in  turn  stimulated  the 
ordinary  accepted  sensory  pathways. 

Helson  (126)  reported  critical  sen.sory  measure- 
ments on  patients  of  Frazier  who  had  undergone 
trigeminal  denervation  of  the  second  and  third  divi- 
sions. He  found  that  such  patients  reacted  violently 
if  a  hot  cylinder  was  kept  on  the  face  more  than  a 
few  seconds.  But  in  three  patients  in  whom  thoracic 
sympathectomy  had  been  added  to  the  trigeminal 
rhizotomy  he  could  sear  the  skin  with  a  hot  cylinder 
and  evoke  only  a  sense  of  pressure.  Hence  he  agreed 
with  Foerster  et  al.  Cyg)  that  the  cervical  sympathetic 
nerves  contain  afferent  fibers. 

During  our  own  stimulations  of  the  sympathetic 
nerves  in  the  neck,  pain  occurred  in  9  of  to  indi- 
viduals, init  it  was  not  elicited  from  all  portions  of 
the  trunk  or  superior  cervical  ganglion  when  small 
bipolar  electrodes  with  points  less  than  i  mm  apart 
were  used,  whereas  an  effective  stimulus  (at  similar 
voltage)  applied  to  almost  any  point  on  a  nerve  of 
the  cervical  plexus  caused  pain.  The  details  of  the 
respon.ses  are  summarized  by  White  &  Sweet  (296, 
pp.  84  to  89);  we  concluded  that  there  must  be  great 
variation  in  the  distribution  of  pain  fibers  within  the 
cervical  sympathetics  in  man  which  would  account 
for  Frazier's  vacillating  opinion  as  to  whether  or  not 
they  were  present  at  all  (82).  However  the  appearance 
of  pain  upon  stimulus  to  central  ends,  either  of  cut 
peripheral  sympathetic  branches  or  of  cut  gray  rami 
communicantes,  made  it  clear  that  true  afferent 
fibers  do  occur  in  them.  Activation  of  efferent  sympa- 
thetic fibers  with  subsequent  conduction  via  cranial 


sensory  fibers  in  nervus  trigeminus  or  intermedins 
could  be  excluded  as  the  mechanism  of  pain  in  such 
instances  and  in  another  patient  in  whom  the  fifth, 
seventh  and  eighth  cranial  nerves  had  been  divided. 

That  the  sensory  inflow  back  to  the  cord  is  not 
confined  to  the  white  rami  communicantes  was  fur- 
ther shown  in  fi\e  other  patients  in  our  series  in 
whom  stimulus  to  each  member  of  one  or  more 
pairs  of  rami  elicited  pain.  In  several  of  these  each 
end  of  each  cut  ramus  was  stimulated;  pain  was 
elicited  only  from  the  end  toward  the  spinal  nerve. 
The  pain  came  immediately  upon  stimulation,  was 
obtained  at  about  the  .same  threshold  and  had  the 
same  reference  as  that  from  the  intact  ramus.  One 
is  unable  to  distinguish  the  white  from  the  gray 
ramus  in  anv  given  pair,  but  our  results  indicate 
that  the  pain  afferents  travellino  with  the  sympathetic 
are  not  restricted  to  the  portals  of  entrance  to  the 
central  nervous  system  u.sed  by  the  efferent  sympa- 
thetic fibers,  i.e.  the  white  rami  from  C8  to  the  upper 
lumbar  area.  Instead  pain  afferents  may  perhaps 
reach  the  spinal  cord  via  any  of  the  gray  or  white 
rami. 

The  presence  of  many  pain  fibers  in  the  cardiac 
and  splanchnic  branches  of  the  sympathetic  trunk 
has  been  widely  demonstrated  by  stimulation.  Can- 
non (38)  buried  electrodes  in  contact  with  the  vagus 
or  splanchnic  nerves  in  cats.  After  the  wound  was 
healed  stimulation  of  the  latter  nerves  made  the 
animals  restless  and  the  presence  of  pain  was  in- 
ferred. Vagal  stimulation  caused  only  respiratory 
effects.  White  et  al.  (295)  thought  they  relieved 
experimental  cardiac  pain  in  dogs  by  resection  of  the 
upper  four  thoracic  ganglia  and  Davis  et  a!.  (54) 
concluded  that  the  pain  of  their  animals  on  disten- 
sion of  the  gall  bladder  was  stopped  by  splanchnicec- 
tomy.  Balchum  &  Weaver  (13)  reached  the  same 
conclusion  regarding  the  pain  of  gastric  distension 
in  the  158  dogs  they  studied.  Leriche  &  Fontaine 
(168)  provoked  pain  in  the  heart  and  precordial 
region  by  stimulation  of  the  lower  pole  of  the  stellate 
ganglion  in  two  patients.  In  a  third  patient  who  had 
never  had  angina  pectoris,  faradization  of  the  stellate 
ganglion  seemed  to  bring  on  an  intense  anginal 
attack.  In  at  least  three  other  individuals  with  clinical 
angina  an  attack  has  been  elicited  during  dissection 
at  the  stellate  ganglion  [Jonnesco  and  Bouchard 
cited  in  (168);  (167,  pp.  375  to  376)].  The  effective- 
ness of  upper  thoracic  sympathectomy  in  eliminating 
afferent  fibers  for  pain  from  the  heart  is  attested  by 
two  large  series  of  patients  relie\ed  thereby  of  severe 


482 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


angina  pectoris,  reported  by  Lindgren  &  Olivecrona 
(176),  and  by  White  &  fiiand  (294). 

Electrical  stimulation  of  the  central  end  of  the  cut 
great  splanchnic  nerves  produced  some  of  the  more 
painful  experiences  seen  in  man  by  Foerster  (77, 
p.  32).  When  the  patient  is  under  spinal  anesthesia 
pain  upon  splanchnic  stimulation  is  referred  some- 
where in  the  chest  above  the  level  of  analgesia, 
according  to  Adson  (3)  and  Leriche  (166).  On  the 
other  hand,  in  patients  in  whom  we  have  stimulated 
the  central  stump  of  the  greater  and  lesser  splanchnic 
nerves  under  local  anesthesia  the  pain  has  always 
been  referred  to  the  ipsilateral  abdomen.  Such  pain 
appeared  at  the  same  low  threshold  without  delay 
and  was  of  about  the  same  intensity  as  that  evoked 
from  the  twelfth  intercostal  nerve  several  centimeters 
lateral  to  the  rami  communicantes  (296,  p.  83). 
Such  pain  also  ensued  upon  stimulation  of  the  twigs 
of  origin  of  the  greater  and  lesser  splanchnic  nerves 
from  the  sympathetic  trunk  and  from  their  rostral 
cut  ends.  From  all  of  these  nerves  no  sensation  oc- 
curred upon  high  voltage  stimulation  of  their  caudal 
cut  ends. 

Bilateral  sympathectomy  from  the  midthoracic 
through  the  third  lumbar  ganglia  and  including 
the  splanchnic  nerves  from  the  T7  ramus  to  beyond 
the  celiac  ganglion  (performed  for  hypertension) 
yielded  a  series  of  patients  for  study  of  abdominal 
visceral  sensation  by  Ray  &  Neill  (221).  They  found 
the  pain  sense  absent  in  these  patients  in  the  stomach, 
intestine  (except  the  rectum),  extrahepatic  biliary 
tract,  pancreas,  kidney  and  ureter.  The  stimuli  for 
pain  included  distension  by  balloons  of  hollow  viscera, 
and  traction  and  faradic  stimulation  of  all  the 
structures  mentioned.  Studies  after  unilateral  sym- 
pathectomy revealed  a  homolateral  afferent  supply 
for  kidney,  ureter,  the  two  sides  of  the  colon  and  pos- 
siblv  the  gastric  mesentery;  the  remaining  organs 
had  a  bilateral  supply.  Bentley  &  Smithwick  (22) 
had  shown  earlier  that  balloon  distension  of  the  duo- 
denum and  jejunum  was  no  longer  painful  after 
thoracolumbar  sympathectomy  and  splanchnicec- 
tomy.  Bentley  (21)  stopped  the  pain  evoked  by 
transfixing  an  exposed  duodenal  ulcer  with  a  needle 
when  he  procainized  the  splanchnic  nerves.  Numer- 
ous other  animal  and  experimental  studies  confirm 
that  the  pain  afTerents  from  the  abdominal  viscera 
travel  with  the  sympathetic  nerves,  and  a  substantial 
number  of  patients  with  pain  arising  in  these  viscera 
have  been  relieved  by  appropriate  sympathectomy 
according  to  White  &  Sweet  (296,  pp.  652  to  676). 

Gernandt   &    Zotterman  (96)  have  made  a  con- 


tribution not  readily  feasible  in  man  by  recording 
oscilloscopically  from  the  splanchnic  nerve  and  from 
fine  strands  of  mesenteric  nerve  in  the  cat.  Slight 
pressure  or  touch  to  the  small  intestine  gave  no  elec- 
trical impulses  but  pinching  the  gut  or  the  mesentery 
produced  delta  fiber  impulses  conducted  at  up  to 
20  m  per  sec.  and  much  slower  impulses  in  'C  fibers 
conducted  at  0.5  to  2  m  per  sec.  These  authors  con- 
cluded that  intestinal  sensibility  is  similar  to  that  of 
skin  deprived  of  its  fast  conducting  afferents. 

In  the  limbs  the  presence  of  afferent  fibers  in  the 
sympathetic  supply  is  less  consistently  demonstrable 
by  stimulation  in  man,  especially  in  the  lower  limb. 
However  Leriche  &  Fontaine  (168),  Foerster  et  al. 
(79)  and  Harris  (119)  all  record  examples  of  pain 
referred  to  the  upper  limb  upon  stimulus  to  the  in- 
ferior cervical  or  stellate  ganglion.  The  author  has 
seen  one  patient  in  whom  electrodes  applied  to  the 
first  and  second  thoracic  ganglia  caused  immediate 
pain  in  the  entire  ipsilateral  arm  and  in  whom  this 
response  recurred  upon  stimulation  of  the  caudal 
end  of  the  sympathetic  trunk  after  section  below  the 
T2  ganglion — evidence  that  direct  afferent  fibers 
were  stimulated.  Similar  evidence  for  the  lower  limb 
has  been  cited  by  Foerster  et  al.  (79,  p.  154)  and  by 
Echlin  (71).  White  &  Sweet  have  never  .succeeded 
in  evoking  pain  in  the  leg  by  stimulation  of  the  lumbar 
sympathetic  trunk  or  rami.  But  the  type  of  pain 
known  as  causalgia  which  may  follow  trauma  to 
nerves  especially  in  the  limbs  is  consistently  stopped 
by  sympathectomy.  This  fact  is  extensively  docu- 
mented in  table  XIV  of  White  &  Sweet  (296,  p.  369). 
A  possible  explanation  other  than  the  elimination  of 
direct  afferent  pathways  in  the  sympathetics  has  been 
suggested  by  Doupe  et  al.  (68),  namely  that  at  the 
site  of  injury  artificial  synapses  appear  permitting 
tonic  efiferent  impulses  in  sympathetic  nerves  to  excite 
somatic  afferents  for  pain.  If  this  is  true  the  fiber 
interaction  phenomenon  of  Granit  et  al.  (109)  has 
major  clinical  significance.  Relevant  also  are  experi- 
ments of  Walker  &  Nulsen  (280).  They  applied  a 
chronic  pull-out  electrode  to  the  sympathetic  chain 
between  the  T2  and  T3  ganglia  and  divided  the 
trunk  below  this  electrode  in  12  patients.  Onh-  in  the 
three  who  had  causalgia  did  any  pain  appear  in  the 
arm  and  hand  on  stimulus  postoperatively.  In  the.se 
three  there  was  a  consistent  pattern  in  which  the 
pain  appeared  only  4  to  20  sec.  after  the  start  of 
stimulus,  usually  a  few  seconds  after  piloerection 
over  the  whole  upper  limb.  Maximal  pain  was  not 
reached  for  15  to  30  sec;  then,  despite  continuation 
of  the  stimulus,  it  slowly  faded  and  disappeared   15 


483 


to  30  sec.  after  its  peak.  Although  the  sequence  of 
events  is  too  slow  to  suggest  fiber  interaction,  efferent 
sympathetic  discharges  had  apparently  set  up  pain 
impulses  at  the  periphery. 

We  are  not  aware  that  any  observer  has  reported 
decreased  appreciation  of  pinprick  in  man  after 
sympathectomy,  but  van  Harreveld  &  .Smith  (272) 
thought  that  extensive  thoracicoabdominal  sympa- 
thectomy produced  additional  loss  of  pain  from  the 
skin  in  seven  of  eight  lower  thoracic  and  upper  lum- 
bar segments  studied  in  the  cat.  They  isolated  a 
dermatome  by  cutting  three  spinal  nerves  above  and 
three  below  the  one  they  left  intact.  The  borders  of 
this  dermatome  as  determined  by  the  motor  respon.se 
to  pinching  proved  constant.  After  the  sympathec- 
tomy there  was  then  an  added  loss  of  .sensitivity  to 
pinching  in  a  small  often  triangular  zone  at  the 
cranioventral  side  of  the  dermatome.  They  sug- 
gested that  these  sympathetic  afferents  might  go  to 
the  blood  vessels  of  the  skin. 


Parasympathetic  Nerves 

Of  the  cranial  autonomic  nerves  we  shall  mention 
only  the  vagus.  That  this  carries  afferent  fibers  from 
the  trachea  and  bronchi  is  suggested  by  the  finding  of 
Morton  et  al.  (193)  who  relieved  the  pain  and  cough 
of  bronchogenic  carcinoma  by  section  of  the  homo- 
lateral vagus  below  its  recurrent  laryngeal  branch. 
The  presence  of  other  afferent  fibers  perhaps  from 
the  thoracic  esophagus  is  intimated  by  the  observa- 
tions of  Grimson  et  al.  (112).  Their  patients  under 
spinal  anesthesia  experienced  'heartburn'  and  pain 
referred  to  the  neck  when  the  vagus  was  stimulated 
three  inches  above  the  diaphragm.  Stimulation  at  or 
below  the  diaphragm  caused  no  pain,  so  the  vagi 
probably  carry  no  such  fibers  from  the  abdominal 
viscera.  This  was  also  the  conclusion  of  Cannon  in 
his  cats  (38). 

The  problem  of  pain  conduction  by  afferent  sacral 
parasympathetic  fibers  is  discussed  by  White  & 
Sweet  (296,  pp.  671  to  674). 

On  balance  it  is  our  impression  that  afferent  fibers 
for  pain  are  to  i)e  found  in  so  many  of  the  autonomic 
nerves  in  man  that  no  useful  purpose  is  served  by 
regarding  these  as  comprising  a  purely  efferent  ner- 
vous system,  the  more  so  since  a  numl:)er  of  the  con- 
siderations which  led  to  the  development  of  this  con- 
cept by  Ga.skell  &  Langley  have  been  shown  to  be 
invalid. 


SPIN.AL  CORD 

Upon  entry  into  the  spinal  cord  the  posterior  root 
filaments  divide  into  a  lateral  bundle  of  fine  fibers 
and  a  medial  bundle  of  large  fibers.  The  small  lateral 
fibers  bifurcate  at  once  into  two  short  branches  one 
of  which  passes  rostrally,  the  other  caudally,  for  a 
few  segments  in  the  dorsolateral  fasciculus  or  zone  of 
Lissauer  (marginal  zone  of  Waldeyer).  Each  branch 
gives  off  collaterals  which  pass  into  the  posterior 
horn,  according  to  Bok  (29,  p.  534).  We  have  re- 
ferred to  the  work  of  Ranson  &.  Billingsley  (219) 
in  cats  which  places  the  pain  fibers  in  the  lateral 
bundle.  Hyndman  (132)  has  contended  that  incision 
into  this  zone  in  man  produces  an  area  of  analgesia 
without  complete  loss  of  touch  sensation.  R.  W.  Rand, 
E.  J.  Penka  and  W.  E.  Stern,  however,  made  in  two 
patients  a  total  of  15  electrolytic  lesions  in  the  zone 
of  Lissauer  and  were  unable  to  detect  any  sensory 
changes  attributable  thereto.  They  made  an  even 
more  extensive  series  of  rostrocaudal  lesions  in  this 
zone  in  a  monkey,  10  in  number,  i  mm  deep  from 
the  C6  through  Ti  cord  segments.  Examination 
post-mortem  showed  the  lesions  extending  but  little 
beyond  the  desired  zone  of  destruction  which  had 
produced  analgesia  only  of  the  ulnar  aspect  of  the 
ipsilateral  forearm  and  hypalgesia  of  the  ulnar  area 
of  the  hand.  Because  of  the  above-mentioned  rostro- 
caudal fanning  of  the  fibers,  it  is  not  surprising  that 
an  extended  continuous  lesion  is  required  to  produce 
any  demonstrable  sensory  loss. 

The  posterior  root  fibers  terminate  around :  a)  the 
posteromarginal  or  perlcornual  cells  which  lie 
around  the  entire  margin  of  the  posterior  horn,  A) 
the  more  centrally  placed  cells  of  the  nucleus  pro- 
prius  of  the  posterior  horn  and  c)  small  cells  lying 
within  the  substantia  gelatinosa  which  caps  the  nu- 
cleus proprius,  as  shown  in  figure  7.  Pearson  (207) 
from  studies  of  his  Golgi  preparations  of  spinal 
cords  of  human  babies  finds  that  these  small  cells 
in  the  substantia  gelatino.sa  may  intervene  between 
some  of  the  primary  afferent  terminals  and  the 
larger  cells  which  lie  in  the  nucleus  proprius  and  in 
the  pericornual  regions.  The  latter  two  groups  of 
cells  give  rise  to  the  major  crossed  ascending  af- 
ferent pathways  in  the  cord.  Pearson  hypothecates 
that  the  primary  afferent  fibers  which  end  directly 
in  relation  to  the.se  latter  cells  would  be  likely  to 
give  rise  to  'fast  pain,'  whereas  those  cells  contain- 
ing a  sinall  intercalated  neuron  of  the  substantia 
gelatinosa  might   be  those  evoking  'slow  pain.' 

The   pericornual   cells   are   middle-sized   ganglion 


484 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7.  Termination  of  pain  fibers  of  the  posterior  root  in 
human  spinal  cord.  Large  fiber  A  passes  through  the  substan- 
tia gelatinosa  to  terminate  near  a  cell  of  nucleus  proprius. 
Small  fiber  C  ends  within  substantia  gelatinosa  in  relation  to 
either:  a  pericornual  cell,  a  cell  of  nucleus  proprius,  or  a  small 
cell  within  the  substantia  gelatinosa.  Long  axons  crossing  in 
the  anterior  commissure  and  ascending  the  cord  arise  from  the 
nucleus  proprius  or  pericornual  group.  [Modified  from  Pear- 
son (207).] 


cells  lying  in  three  main  groups  around  the  margin 
of  the  posterior  horn,  a  special  group  at  its  dorsal 
tip,  a  reticular  or  lateral  group  adjoining  the  pos- 
terolateral column  of  white  matter  and  an  inner 
medial  group  adjoining  the  posterior  white  column. 
These  extend  the  full  length  of  the  cord,  and  Kuru 
concludes  that  they  give  rise  to  the  pain  and  tem- 
perature fibers  (149,  pp.  10  and  11).  He  has  studied 
the  retrograde  cell  degeneration  in  the  posterior 
horns  of  patients  who  have  had  anterolateral  cor- 
dotomy, with  attention  to  those  who  have  had  in- 
complete analgesia  and  thermanesthesia  below  the 
expected  level.  He  found  a  striking  correlation  be- 
tween the  segments  of  the  cord  showing  chromatoly- 
sis  of  the  pericornual  cells  and  the  dermal  segments 
showing  loss  of  pain  and  temperature  sensation.  The 
patients  in  whose  pericornual  cells  below  the  level 
of  operation  he  saw  the  greatest  chromatolysis  also 
had  the  greatest  degeneration  in  the  lateral  spino- 
thalamic tract.  The  larger  cells  occupying  the  more 


centrally  placed  nucleus  proprius  of  the  posterior 
horn  were  not  degenerated  unless  the  anterior  white 
columns  were  cut.  They  did  show  chromatolysis, 
however,  in  three  patients  whose  incisions  included 
the  anterior  white  matter.  In  the.se  three  patients  he 
traced  degeneration  in  a  distinct  ventral  spino- 
thalamic tract,  which  he  did  not  associate  with  loss 
of  pain  and  temperature. 

Anatomical  details  as  to  the  number  of  segments 
required  for  crossover  of  the  pain  fibers  through  the 
anterior  commi.s.sure,  the  likelihood  of  some  fiJDers 
remaining  uncrossed  and  the  consistency  with  which 
fibers  from  a  given  area  of  the  body  occupy  a  par- 
ticular portion  of  the  opposite  anterior  quadrant 
of  the  cord  are  of  great  importance  to  the  surgeon 
seeking  to  relieve  pain  but  need  not  engross  us  here. 
White  &  Sweet  (296,  pp.  37  to  45)  may  be  consulted 
for  details. 

Although  the  association  of  pain  fibers  with  tho.se 
for  temperature  in  the  anterior  white  quadrant  is  a 
close  one  there  are  a  number  of  recorded  results  of 
dissociation  of  loss  of  pain  from  that  of  temperature. 
Analgesia  to  pinprick  with  little  or  no  thermanes- 
thesia was  recorded  following  small,  fractionally 
enlarged  incisions  for  anterolateral  cordotomy  by 
Wilson  &  Fay  (299)  in  one  of  two  cases,  by  Stookey 
(255)  in  four  cases,  by  Grant  (no)  in  one  case  and 
by  Foerster  &  Gagel  (80)  and  Kuru  (149)  in  3  of  30 
cases.  The  converse  state  of  severe  hypothermesthesia 
with  preservation  of  normal  pain  sense  has  also 
been  recorded  by  Frazier  &  Spiller  (83)  in  a  pa- 
tient with  a  midcervical  cord  tumor.  The  paucity 
of  .such  observations  indicates  that  there  usually  are 
not  two  distinct  bundles  of  fibers  for  pain  and  tem- 
perature, but  the  fact  that  such  dissociation  can  oc- 
cur suggests  that  individual  fibers  are  concerned 
with  impulses  either  for  pain  or  for  temperature. 
Such  a  thought  is  further  intimated  by  the  observa- 
tion of  Sweet  (259)  that  bipolar  electrical  stimula- 
tion within  the  anterior  half  of  the  cord  in  man 
elicited  responses  purely  of  temperature  (usually 
heat)  in  46  per  cent  of  the  responses  in  which  any 
subjective  sensation  occurred.  The  usual  response 
was  one  of  pain  with  or  without  a  burning  quality 
in  54  per  cent  of  200  stimulations 

A  number  of  factors  have  conspired  to  make 
physiologic  studies  of  pain  in  the  central  nervous 
system  even  more  difficult  than  those  of  the  periph- 
eral somatic  and  autonomic  systems.  As  mentioned 
earlier,  pain  pathways  in  the  cords  of  animals  ap- 
pear to  be  more  difTusely  distributed  and  to  ascend 
by  multiple  relays  with  cro.ssing  and   recros.sing  of 


PAIN 


485 


fibers.  Furthermore,  although  the  experience  of 
pain  is  a  more  compelling  usurper  of  conscious  at- 
tention and  tends  to  evoke  more  obvious  motor 
activity  than  touch,  pressure  or  the  movement  of  a 
limb,  these  latter  stimuli  are  accompanied  by  far 
more  conspicuous  signs  of  electrical  activity  appear- 
ing at  a  lower  threshold.  This  is  of  course  due  to  the 
fact  that  the  latter  modalities  tend  to  traverse  larger 
fibers  and  the  recorded  potential  is  in  approximately 
linear  relation  to  the  diameter  of  the  fiber,  as  shown 
by  Gasser  &  Grundfest  (92).  Hence  but  few  workers 
have  carried  their  studies  into  the  difiicult  realm  of 
analysis  of  the  smaller  late  potentials  from  noxious 
stimuli. 

There  are  several  facts  which  indicate  that  pain 
impulse  conduction  within  the  cord  ma\'  invoke 
unmyelinated   as  well   as   myelinated   fibers.    In   the 


painstaking  study  of  Haggqvist  (i  14)  17,000  fibers  in 
a  single  cross  section  of  a  young  woman's  cord  at 
the  T3  .segment  were  measured.  Samples  were 
counted  from  each  of  the  zones  numbered  in  figure 
8.  Forty-two  per  cent  of  all  the  fibers  in  the  whole 
cross  section  measured  2  ixor  less  in  diameter,  whereas 
in  the  anterolateral  zones  6  and  7,  the  division  of 
which  would  yield  contralateral  analgesia,  55  per 
cent  and  61  per  cent,  respectively,  of  the  fibers  were 
of  this  small  diameter. 

Most  of  the  histologic  studies  of  fiber  tracts  in- 
cluding those  for  pain  have  relied  on  myelin  or  its 
degeneration  products,  but  it  would  be  a  coincidence 
if  the  unmyelinated  pain  pathways  were  coexten- 
sive with  pathways  we  can  see  in  such  stains  as  the 
Swank-Davenport  (258)  modification  of  the  Marchi 
method.  And  indeed  we  shall  see  that  in  the  brain 


.  .y?^. 


1    i    M    S    6    T    S    t    t&    11   a  II  n   IS  ti  17  IS    //  20  Vfi 


FIG.  8.  Fiber  sizes  in  the  spinal  cord.  Cross  section  at  T3  segment  in  which  Haggqvist  measured 
diameters  of  17,000  fibers  and  subdivided  the  white  matter  into  14  zones  on  the  basis  of  differing 
constellations  of  fiber  sizes.  The  histogram  below  and  to  left  indicates  the  percentage  of  fibers  of 
each  diameter  in  the  entire  hemisection.  In  the  regions  ventral  and  ventromedial  to  the  anterior 
horn  fibers  less  than  2  n  constitute  43  to  45  per  cent  of  total.  Since  this  is  about  the  general  average, 
these  studies  give  no  clue  that  the  pain  fibers  lying  here  preponderate  over  tiny  fibers  with  other 
functions.  See  te.xt  for  different  deductions  with  respect  to  zones  6  and  7.  [From  Haggqvist  (i  14).] 


486 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Stem  the  position  of  the  pain  pathways  correlates 
poorly  with  the  position  of  Marchi  degeneration  in 
spinotectal  and  spinothalamic  fibers. 

Impulses  from  somatic  nerves  were  traced  into 
the  anterolateral  column  of  the  cat's  spinal  cord  by 
Collins  &  Randt  (49).  They  studied  with  10  m  tip 
microelectrodes  the  responses  evoked  from  stimula- 
tion of  contralateral  sciatic  or  superficial  radial 
nerves,  and  compared  these  with  the  responses  to  be 
seen  in  the  ipsilateral  dorsal  column.  Velocities  of 
impulses  were  compared  for  the  two  loci  both  in  the 
peripheral  nerve  and  in  the  cord.  The  responses  in 
the  anterolateral  quadrant  were  related  to  the 
gamma  component  in  the  nerve.  In  the  peripheral 
nerve  they  traveled  at  about  34  m  per  sec;  in  the 
cord  they  averaged  24.9  m  per  sec.  ranging  from  19 
to  33  m  per  sec.  The  dorsal  column  impulses  while 
in  the  peripheral  nerve  traveled  at  about  85  m  per 
sec.  and  slowed  down  to  an  average  of  50.5  m  per 
sec.  in  the  cord.  The  typical  anterolateral  potential 
had  slow  rising  and  falling  phases  with  superimposed 
spike  activity  which  was  maximal  at  the  peak  of 
the  slower  potential.  The  maximum  voltages  were 
from  50  to  75  Mv,  the  total  duration  was  30  to  40 
msec.  The  ratios  of  threshold  potentials  in  the 
anterolateral  column  to  the  dorsal  column  were 
2.6:1  for  a  5  msec,  shock,  corresponding  closely  to 
the  ratios  of  these  potentials  in  peripheral  nerve  of 
2.4:1.  The  anterolateral  column  potentials  traverse 
this  portion  of  the  cord  throughout  its  length  as 
shown  by  their  abolition  in  a  cervical  lead  following 
thoracic  anterolateral  section  (which  leaves  intact 
the  cervical  dorsal  column  potential). 

Impulses  from  autonomic  nerves  were  traced  into 
the  anterolateral  column  of  the  cord  of  rabbits,  cats 
and  dogs  by  Amassian  (5).  He  recorded  with  micro- 
electrodes  the  responses  excited  by  stimulation  of  the 
splanchnic  nerve.  When  this  was  increased  to  15  v. 
with  a  o.  I  msec,  shock  a  large  fraction  of  'A'  gamma- 
delta  fibers  in  the  nerve  was  excited.  A  barrage  of 
spikes  could  then  be  seen  bilaterally  in  the  antero- 
lateral region  of  the  cord  clo.se  to  the  gray  matter. 
Its  long  latency  of  1 1  to  13  msec,  and  much  greater 
duration  of  25  msec,  distinguished  it  from  the  pos- 
terior column  wave.  There  is  a  striking  similarity 
between  these  potentials  and  those  recorded  after 
stimulation  of  somatic  nerves  from  almost  the  same 
spots  in  the  cord,  the  splanchnic  responses  a  bit 
medial  to  those  from  the  somatic  nerves.  The  high 
voltage  required  provoked  reflex  movements  of  the 
body  wall,  but  such  motor  actixity  did  not  then  set 
up  the  whole  potential  in  the  anterolateral  column 


because  this  was  only  partially  reduced  when  the 
movements  were  stopped  with  </-tubocurarine. 
Amassian  is  appropriately  cautious  about  correlat- 
ing this  pathway  with  that  subserving  visceral  pain 
in  man.  Both  clinical  and  experimentally  induced 
pain  in  gastrointestinal  and  urinary  viscera  is  usually 
stopped  in  patients  by  anterolateral  cordotomy  on 
the  side  opposite  a  laterally  placed  viscus.  So  the 
crossover  of  splanchnic  pain  fibers  appears  to  be 
more  complete  in  man  than  the  fibers  from  which 
Ama.ssian  was  recording.  Visceral  aff"erent  path- 
ways in  the  cat  had  already  been  shown  to  be  in- 
completely crossed  by  Spiegel  &  Bernis  (253)  who 
found  that  stimulation  of  the  central  end  of  one 
splanchnic  nerve  caused  'pain  responses'  until  both 
anterolateral  columns  were  destroyed. 

The  following  evidence  indicates  that  impulses  for 
pain  may  not  ascend  the  cord  exclusively  via  a  single 
fiber  running  in  the  anterior  or  anterolateral  white 
matter  to  reach  the  brain  stem,  a)  Vigorous  stimula- 
tion, as  with  bipolar  electrodes  at  100  or  more  v., 
consistently  causes  pain  in  an  area  apparently  de- 
nervated  by  full  anterior  quadrant  section  as  judged 
by  analgesia  to  pinprick  and  to  a  variety  of  other 
forms  of  experimentally  induced  pain  (296,  p.  45). 
King's  (141)  careful  measurements  revealed  that 
threshold  voltage  values  for  pricking  pain  on  the 
analgesic  side  were  only  40  to  50  per  cent  greater 
than  on  the  normal  side.  That  the  pain  impulses  are 
not  entering  the  cord  at  levels  above  the  cordotomy 
incision,  having  moved  rostrally  along  sympathetic 
pathways  in  the  paravertebral  trunks  or  along  the 
aorta,  is  reasonably  certain  because  the  finding  is 
the  same  following  high  cervical  cordotomy.  6) 
Direct  bipolar  electrical  stimulation  applied  to  the 
surface  of  the  posterior  and  posterolateral  columns 
of  the  cord  in  man  causes  severe  tingling  sensations 
like  an  electric  shock.  Foerster  &  Gagel  (80)  de- 
scribed such  responses;  we  confirm  that  at  low 
thresholds  (of  less  than  o.oi  v.  in  our  hands)  applica- 
tion of  the  electrodes  to  the  fasciculus  cuneatus 
causes  reference  to  the  ipsilateral  leg  or  pelvis,  and 
to  the  fasciculus  gracilis  causes  reference  to  the 
ipsilateral  arm.  At  higher  thresholds  one  evokes 
similar  responses  contralaterally  from  the  surface  of 
the  posterolateral  column  of  white  matter.  That 
such  pathways  are  rarely  used  in  pain  of  clinical 
cause  is  clear  from  the  high  percentage  of  patients 
relieved  of  pain  by  anterior  quadrant  section.  Pos- 
sibly these  observations  have  in  fact  no  physiologic 
significance,  and  electrical  stimulus  to  the  exposed 
spinal  cord  may  be  a  condition  for  which  there  is  no 


PAIN 


487 


physiopathologic  counterpart.  <)  That  the  dorsal 
columns  may,  however,  even  if  only  rarely,  carry 
impulses  causing  clinical  pain  seems  a  tenable  hy- 
pothesis from  the  results  of  Browder  &  Gallagher 
(35).  Their  operative  division  of  the  dorsal  column 
relieved,  in  three  of  four  patients,  pain  referred  to  a 
phantom  lower  limb  which  seemed  to  be  in  a  dis- 
torted posture.  Moreover,  tingling  sensations  per- 
haps like  those  evoked  on  posterior  column  stimula- 
tion may  occur  in  the  analgesic  limb  after  cordotomy 
upon  an  unusually  no.xious  event,  such  as  running 
a  nail  into  the  foot. 


.MEDULLA    OBLONGATA 

The  primary  afferent  neurons  for  pain  and  tem- 
perature arising  from  the  face  and  head  via  tri- 
geminal, nervus  intermedius,  glossopharyngeal  and 
vagal  routes  collect  in  the  descending  or  spinal 
trigeminal  tract  and  terminate  near  cells  in  the 
lower  part  of  the  nucleus  of  that  tract.  The  cells  in 


this  position  extending  froin  about  the  obex  down- 
ward were  called  by  Winkler  the  nucleus  gelatinosus 
tractus  spinalis  (302,  pp.  51  to  59)  because  they 
resemble  those  of  the  substantia  gelatinosa  Rolandi 
of  the  spinal  cord.  Olszewski's  (202^  more  recent 
careful  study  of  the  nucleus  in  man  and  monkev  is 
in  general  agreement.  Section  of  the  descending 
tract  at  about  the  level  of  the  obex  usually  produces 
trigeminal  analgesia  as  well  as  severe  hypalgesia  of 
the  deeper  areas  of  the  face  and  head  supplied  by 
the  afferent  fibers  in  the  seventh,  ninth  and  tenth 
cranial  nerves;  so  the  correlation  of  the  'subnucleus 
gelatinosus'  of  Olszewski  with  pain  and  temperature 
function  seems  likely.  Evidence  on  these  points  as 
well  as  on  the  finer  details  of  topographic  localization 
of  the  fibers  from  various  portions  of  the  head  and 
face  within  the  tract  and  their  termination  in  the 
nucelus  are  summarized  by  White  &  Sweet  (296,  pp. 
457  to  466). 

The  locus  of  spinothalamic  fibers  ascending  from 
the  secondary  afferent  neinons  of  the  cord  as  de- 
termined by  Marchi  stain  is  illustrated  in  figure  9. 


4lh    Ventricle 


Bulbo- 

T  ha  1 om I 

Troct 


Spinocerebellar 
Troct 


Lot  erol 

Spi  notholo  mic 

Troct 


Mediol  Lcnniicoi 


nttrior  Oliv* 


FIG.  9.  Degeneration  in  the  spinothalamic  and  bulbothalamic  tracts  at  level  of  the  inferior  olive. 
The  Marchi  degeneration  in  the  lateral  spinothalamic  tract  (including  spinotectal  fibers)  is  that 
seen  by  Kuru  in  a  patient  all  of  whose  pain  fibers  in  the  anterior  half  of  the  cord  below  C4  segment 
had  degenerated.  We  show  the  locus  of  the  bulbothalamic  tract  as  that  area  of  absence  of  Weigert- 
stained  fibers  described  by  Wallenberg  in  a  patient  who  had  post-mortem  a  softening  in  the  ventral 
two-thirds  of  the  descending  trigeminal  tract  and  its  nucleus.  The  ictus  had  occurred  5  years  earlier; 
the  infarct  it  produced  was  of  maximal  size  at  the  level  of  the  obex,  i.e.  about  the  rostral  end  of  the 
nucleus  for  pain  fibers.  We  have  referred  to  the  secondary  afferent  pathway  from  this  area  as  the 
bulbothalamic  rather  than  trigeminothalamic  tract  since  it  probably  includes  the  area  of  nervus 
intermedius,  glossopharyngcus  and  vagus  as  well  as  trigeminus.  [Modified  from  Kuru  (149).] 


488 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYS!OL(jnV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


The  figures  of  other  workers,  such  as  Goldstein  (103), 
Foerster  Sc  Gagel  (80,  p.  24),  Walker  (276),  Rasmus- 
sen  &  Peyton  (220),  Gardner  &  Cuneo  (87)  and 
Poirier  &  Bertrand  (213),  are  in  general  agreement. 
Although  such  degeneration  has  provided  a  subpial 
signpost  to  the  localization  of  the  pain  fibers,  it  has 
failed  utterly  to  intimate  their  full  extent.  This  was 
first  shown  by  clinical  plus  post-mortem  studies  of 
the  lesions  after  thrombosis  of  vessels  supplying  the 
bulbar  brain  stem.  These  deductions  were  fully 
confirmed  by  the  pioneering  surgical  work  in  man  of 
Schwartz  &  O'Leary  (242,  243^,  and  of  White  (293) 
and  by  subsequent  surgeons,  Crawford  (50)  and 
D'Errico  (64).  Findings  with  respect  to  pain  pro- 
voked by  stimulation  at  operation  were  checked 
against  depth  of  incision,  postoperative  analgesia 
and,  at  times,  later  post-mortem  studies.  These  show 
that  the  pain  fibers  coming  up  from  the  cord  occupy 
a  much  wider  area  just  dorsal  to  the  inferior  olives 
extending  6  to  7  mm  deep  and  continuing  medially 
in  the  midst  of  the  bulbar  reticular  formation  nearly 
to  the  medial  lemniscus.  The  quintoth^lamic  or 
secondary  afferent  trigeminal  fibers  tend  to  lie  in 
the  more  medial  part  of  this  area  and  to  extend  more 
dorsally  as  well  (D'Errico).  McKinley  &  Magoun 
(186)  have  shown  from  depth  recording  of  action 
potentials  in  cats  that  there  is  indiscriminate  mixing 
of  the  fibers  from  the  three  trigeminal  divisions  in 
this  area,  whereas  the  grouping  of  fibers  related  to 
trigeminal  peripheral  divisions  is  clearer  in  the  de- 
scending trigeminal  tract  and  nucleus,  as  shown  by 
McKinley  &  Magoun  (186)  and  Harrison  &  Corbin 
(120).  Subsequent  work  in  man  has  also  shown  that 
a  discernible  tendency  to  layering  in  the  cord  of 
the  fibers  from  specific  sections  of  the  body  becomes 
less  consistent  in  the  medulla. 

More  work  is  especially  necessary  on  the  course 
of  pain  fibers  from  face  and  head  once  thev  start 
up  the  brain  stem  There  is  some  evidence  from 
Wallenberg  (281)  that  these  fibers  separate  into 
two  deep  bundles  as  they  move  rostralh'.  To  the 
illustration  from  Kuru  (fig,  g)  has  been  added  an 
indication  of  Wallenberg's  notion  of  the  location  of 
the.se  secondary  fibers  from  the  face  at  the  mid- 
bulbar  level. 

Numerous  fibers  ascending  from  the  cord  move 
medially  to  terminate  in  the  reticular  formation  of 
pons  and  medulla;  their  possible  significance  will  i^e 
considered  in  the  next  section. 


MESENCEPH.\LON 

In  the  upper  pons  and  midbrain  pain  fibers  again 
become  more  superficial  and  hence  more  accessible 
to  special  analysis  and  surgical  section  in  animals 
and  man.  Here  their  precise  extent  and  location  is 
less  well-known  than  in  the  cord  and  medulla  be- 
cause of  the  smaller  numbers  of  studies.  In  general, 
the  fibers  occupy  a  zone  extending  dorsallv  and 
medially  for  about  i  cm  from  the  lateral  messn- 
cephalic  sulcus.  One  example  will  suffice  to  indicate 
some  of  the  unresol\-ed  discrepancies.  Walker  (278), 
the  major  pioneer  in  this  field,  following  a  trigeminal 
lesion  in  the  monkey  places  the  Marchi  degenera- 
tion in  the  lower  midluain  in  a  narrow  zone  1  to  2 
mm  deep  beginning  right  at  the  surface  and  ex- 
tending dorsally  a  few  millimeters  from  the  lateral 
mesencephalic  sulcus  (fig.  10).  Wallenberg  (281) 
and  van  Gehuchten  (271)  working  with  the  same 
method  in  raijbits  found  the  degeneration  exclu- 
sively in  a  much  more  medial  position,  and  Wallen- 
berg confirmed  his  impression  in  studies  of  degenera- 
tion in  a  patient  (281).  Moreover  the  spinothalamic 
tract  demonstrable  in  Marchi  stains  at  the  level  of 
the  superior  coUiculus  in  man  has  dwindled  to  a 
tiny  bundle.  Having  identified  the  bundle  in  Marchi 
stains,  Glees  &  Bailey  (99)  then  counted  the  fibers 
in  this  region  in  normal  Weigert  preparations;  they 
found  onl\  aijout  1500  fibers.  Of  these  two-thirds 
were  2  to  4  yu  in  diameter;  most  of  the  remainder 
measured  about  4  to  6  /x;  they  were  all  in  a  small 
compact  group  only  aijout  0.65  mm-  in  cross-sec- 
tion. 

However,  figure  10  also  illustrates  the  area  of 
surgical  destruction  in  the  largest  lesion  figured  by 
Walker  (278)  which  did  not  produce  complete 
analgesia  on  the  opposite  face  and  lower  limb  (al- 
though it  did  on  the  torso  and  upper  limb).  Yet  the 
lesion  essentially  blankets  all  of  the  variously  de- 
scribed zones  of  niNelin  degeneration.  It  is  again 
apparent  that  we  need  to  know  more  about  the 
unmyelinated  fibers  and  perhaps  about  the  role  of 
relays   of  neurons   in   conduction   of  pain   impulses. 

The  marked  decrease  in  size  at  the  upper  mid- 
brain of  the  Marchi-stained  ascending  afferent 
bundle  following  extensive  cordotomy  has  long 
been  shown  to  be  due  to  departure  from  it  of  ventral 
spinocerebellar,  spinoreticular  and  spinotectal  fibers. 
The  earlier  descriptions,  such  as  those  of  Foerster  & 
Gagel  (80),  were  confirmed  by  Morin  el  al.  (190) 
who  were  the  first  to  suggest  that  the  spinoreticular 


PAIN  489 


Beginning  of  Brochium 
of    Inf.  Coliiculus 


Lot.  Sp 


Nucleus  of  Inf.  Coliiculus 


FIG.  I  o.  Pain  pathways  in  the  mesencephalon.  On  the  left  side  are  shown :  the  lateral  spinotlialamic 
(inchiding  spinotectal)  tracts  as  seen  in  Marchi  degeneration  after  thoracic  cordotomy  in  man  by 
Rasmussen  &  Peyton  (220),  Gardner  &  Cuneo  (87)  and  Glees  C98);  and  the  bulbothalamic  tract 
as  seen  by  absence  of  Weigert  stained  fibers  seen  following  infarct  by  Wallenberg  (281,  legend  to 
fig.  1 2).  On  the  right  side  are  shown,  diagonal  hatching,  secondary  afferent  pathways  in  monkey  as 
seen  in  Marchi  degeneration :  the  upper  medial  area  after  mid-line  myelotomy  at  L5  to  7 ;  the  lower 
lateral  area  after  lesion  in  spinal  trigeminal  nucleus  according  to  Walker  (278).  Crosshatching:  Lesion 
in  man  which  produced  contralaterally  a  severe  hypalgesia  to  pin  prick  on  face,  analgesia  on  upper 
limb  and  torso  and  hyperpathia  on  lower  limb  (277). 


component  might  influence  the  perception  of  pain  by 
effecting  changes  in  cortical  excitabiUty  via  the 
reticular  formation.  Moruzzi  &  Magoun  (194)  had 
shortly  before  demonstrated  the  widespread  cortical 
activation  from  electrical  stiinulation  in  the  ventro- 
medial bulbar  reticular  formation.  Now  that  stains 
for  axonal  degeneration  are  available  Mehler  el  al. 
(188)  have  shown  in  monkeys,  following  antero- 
lateral cordotomy,  that  there  is  indeed  "a  massive 
fine-fibered,  diffuse,  medial  spinoreticular  system" 
passing  to  1 1  of  the  nuclei  in  the  pontobulbar  reticu- 
lar formation.  They  also  saw  in  the  midijrain  fine 
spinotectal  fibers  passing  to  the  lateral  part  of  the 
central  gray  inatter,  the  nucleus  intcrcoUicularis  and 
deep  strata  of  the  superior  coliiculus.  Bowsher  (31) 
studied  four  patients  following  thoracic,  cervical  or 
bulbar  division   of  pain   pathways   using   the   silver 


stains  for  axonal  degeneration  either  of  Glees  (97) 
or  of  Nauta  &  Gygax  (199).  His  results  show  a  strik- 
ing similarity  to  those  of  the  previous  authors  in 
monkeys. 

Perhaps  these  spinotectal  fibers,  or  bulbothalamic 
continuation  paths  from  spinoreticular  fibers,  are 
responsible  for  a  sharp  spike  potential  found  in  the 
medial  inidbrain  by  Collins  &  O'Leary  (48).  When 
they  stimulated  the  sciatic  or  superficial  radial  nerve 
in  cats,  they  evoked  the  potential  in  a  discrete  region 
of  the  reticular  substance  dorsal  to  the  rostral  part 
of  the  red  nucleus  and  lateral  to  the  oculomotor 
nucleus.  A  relationship  to  pain  was  intimated  by 
these  facts:  a)  the  potential  was  activated  from 
peripheral  axons  of  the  gamma-delta  group,  the 
fastest  of  which  were  conducting  at  45  m  per  sec. ; 
A)  its  pathway  was  principally  \'ia  the  ventrolateral 


490 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


sector  of  the  cord  contralateral  to  the  nerves;  c)  it 
was  increased  in  amplitude  by  such  painful  maneu- 
vers as  spinal  root  manipulation;  and  a')  it  was 
promptly  depressed  by  a  deepening  of  anesthesia 
toward  a  surgical  level. 

By  his  special  technique  of  stimulation  Delgado 
(62)  has  also  obtained  in  monkeys  evidence  that  the 
inferomedial  part  of  the  mesencephalic  central  gray 
matter  is  concerned  with  pain.  The  lateral  part  of 
the  mesencephalic  tegmentum  in  the  region  occupied 
by  the  spinothalamic  tract  and  trigeminal  lemniscus 
has  yielded  similar  responses.  Delgado  has  im- 
planted tiny  multilead  electrodes  and  then  stimu- 
lated via  external  leads  from  these  after  the  wound 
is  closed  and  the  animal  is  relatively  free  to  move 
about.  At  the  aforementioned  points  stimulation 
evokes  the  same  sort  of  complex  response  that  the 
normal  monkey  makes  to  a  peripheral  annoyance 
such  as  pinching  his  tail.  Moreover  in  monkeys  in 
which  such  responses  had  been  elicited  electrically 
the  animal  developed  "conditioned  anxiety,'  i.e.  as 
soon  as  placed  on  the  stimulation  stage  he  screeched, 
bit  and  tried  to  escape.  This  did  not  happen  in  mon- 
keys in  which  purely  somatic  motor  or  autonomic 
effects  had  been  elicited.  Hence  Delgado  assumed 
that  the  sensations  evoked  from  the  mesencephalic 
zones  were  painful  and  were  remembered. 

The  possible  significance  of  these  pathways  may 
be  considered  in  relation  to  curious  sensory  changes 
which  rarely  appear  following  thoracic  or  cervical 
cordotomy,  perhaps  more  often  after  bulbar  spino- 
thalamic tractotomy  for  pain  and  in  many  patients 
after  mesencephalic  tractotomy.  Dogliotti  (67),  the 
first  surgeon  to  divide  pain  pathways  in  the  mid- 
brain, reported  that  his  three  surviving  patients  had 
"diffused  disagreeable  sensations"  in  the  half  of  the 
body  contralateral  to  the  incision.  As  described  by 
Drake  &  McKenzie  (69),  in  all  six  of  their  patients 
after  the  operation  in  the  midbrain  there  was  anal- 
gesia and  thermanesthesia  throughout  the  opposite 
side  of  the  body  and  head  for  3  to  15  days  after 
operation.  Then  pinprick,  deep  pressure  or  thermal 
stimuli  in  all  six  patients  and  even  light  touch  in 
one  of  them  caused  deep  diffuse  poorly-localized 
agonizing  pain  with  strong  withdrawal  and  grimac- 
ing. In  three  of  the  patients  there  was  spontaneous 
burning  pain  in  some  part  of  the  formerly  analgesic 
area.  Drake  suggests  that  the  impulses  causing  these 
pains  traverse  a  secondary  route  via  relay  in  the 
reticular  formation  which  is  not  cut  by  the  incision 
in  the  midbrain.  Walker  (279)  had  already  noted 
"the  diffuse,  disagreeable  sensation  which  may  be 


elicited  by  cold,  extreme  heat  or  pinprick,  especially 
by  repeated  stimulation"  in  some  of  his  patients 
after  mesencephalic  tractotomy.  He  suggested  that 
spinotectal  tracts  may  be  carrying  such  painful 
impulses  to  higher  centers.  Bowsher  (31)  also  pointed 
out  that  bulbothalamic  and  tegmentothalamic  tracts 
running  in  the  reticular  formation  are  separate  from 
direct  spinothalamic  fibers  between  the  level  of  the 
inferior  olive  and  the  thalamus.  He  suggests  that  the 
direct  spinothalamic  system  transmits  impulses  for 
pain  which  is  felt  at  once,  is  sharply  localized  and 
does  not  outlast  the  stimulus.  He  attributes  to  the 
medially  placed  .spinoreticulothalamic  system  the 
diffuse  poorly-localized  pain  with  an  appreciably 
slower  conduction  time  which  does  outlast  the  stimu- 
lus. Since  mesencephalic  incisions  in  man  have 
missed  these  fibers,  this  explanation  would  account 
for  the  type  of  persistent  pain  shown  by  such  patients. 
One  further  observation  of  Drake  &  McKenzie 
also  fits  in  with  this  concept.  One  of  their  patients 
preoperatively  had  had  severe  pain  in  the  face. 
Mesencephalic  tractotomy  replaced  the  original 
pain  by  a  diffuse  facial  burning  sensation.  Division 
of  all  of  the  primary  pain  pathways  from  the  face 
by  bulbar  trigeminal  tractotomy  then  gave  complete 
relief — perhaps  because  reticulothalamic  pathways 
could  no  longer  be  activated. 


TH.'^L.AMUS 

The  fibers  for  touch  and  proprioception  in  the 
medial  lemnisci  mix  with  those  for  pain  and  tem- 
perature as  they  all  terminate  at  the  thalamic  level. 
In  man  when  vascular  lesions  destroy  the  nucleus 
ventralis  posterolateralis  severe  sensory  loss  is  found 
in  the  contralateral  limbs  and  trunk;  the  facial 
fibers  terminate  in  the  nucleus  ventralis  postero- 
medialis  (200,  229}.  These  inferences  from  human 
material  have  been  confirmed  and  extended  by  the 
more  critical  studies  on  Marchi  material  in  lower 
primates  carried  out  by  Clark  (43)  and  Walker 
(275,  pp.  63  to  93).  Moreover  Walker's  (275,  p. 
1 72)  observations  using  the  same  technique  have 
revealed  that  these  same  thalamic  nuclei  project  in 
corresponding  fashion  to  the  postcentral  gyrus  of 
the  same  cerebral  hemisphere.  The  nucleus  ventralis 
posteromedialis  sends  fibers  to  the  lowest  or  facial 
sector  of  the  postcentral  gyrus,  and  the  most  lateral 
parts  of  the  nucleus  ventralis  posterior  project  to  the 
superior  part  of  the  gyrus. 

Foerster    &    Gagel    (80),    Rasmussen    &    Peyton 


491 


(220}  as  well  as  Gardner  &  Cuneo  (87)  have  been 
able  to  follow  only  a  few  degenerating  spinothalamic 
fibers  beyond  the  midbrain  and  into  the  thalamic 
nucleus  ventralis  posterolateralis  after  thoracic 
cordotomy  in  man.  By  the  same  Marchi  method 
Glees  (98)  insists  that  the  degeneration  after  such  an 
operation  is  not  in  the  posteroventral  portion  of  the 
lateral  nucleus  but  dorsal  to  it;  he  sees  the  terminat- 
ing fibers  lying  close  to  the  nucleus  lateralis  posterior. 

Using  stains  for  a.xonal  degeneration  Mehler  (187) 
has  found  that  true  spinothalamic  fibers  to  the  nu- 
cleus ventralis  posterolateralis  constitute  30  per  cent 
of  the  ascending  afferent  fibers  in  the  chimpanzee. 
Moreover  he  also  saw  terminations  in  these  thalamic 
nuclei :  parafascicularis,  paracentralis  and  the  small- 
celled  component  of  the  nucleus  centralis  lateralis. 
Bowsher  (31)  studying  four  patients  following 
thoracic,  cervical  or  bulbar  spinothalamic  tractotomy 
by  either  the  a.xonal  degeneration  stain  of  Glees  or 
that  of  Nauta  found  'a  large  amount  of  degenera- 
tion' in  the  nucleus  ventralis  posterolateralis  on  the 
side  of  the  surgical  lesion  as  well  as  a  little  in  the 
same  nucleus  on  the  other  side.  The  fibers  reach  the 
contralateral  thalamus  by  way  of  the  dorsal  part  of 
the  posterior  commissure.  Moreover  Bowsher  found 
terminations  in  the  relatively  large  nucleus  centrum 
medianum  of  man  not  hitherto  described  even  in 
lower  primates.  He  also  saw  a  few  degenerating  fibers 
in  the  rostral  part  of  the  thalamic  reticular  nucleus. 

Although  there  are  numerous  careful  studies  of 
thalamic  action  potentials  evoked  by  electrical 
stimulation  of  afferent  nerves  these  have  been  cor- 
related mainly  with  touch  and  have  dealt  largely 
with  the  potentials  conducted  by  the  fastest  fibers 
presumably  related  to  touch  or  proprioception. 
However  Gaze  &  Gordon  (93,  94)  recorded  simul- 
taneously the  electrical  activity  of  single  neural 
units  in  the  thalamus  and  the  compoimd  action  po- 
tential from  the  saphenous  nerve  after  stimulation 
of  this  uncut  nerve  in  the  cat  and  monkey.  Having 
found  an  active  thalamic  unit,  the  investigators 
then  sought  to  determine  the  form  of  cutaneous 
stimulus  which  would  cause  it  to  fire.  The  units 
responding  to  electrical  stimulus  of  alpha,  beta  or 
gamma  saphenous  nerve  fibers  usually  responded 
also  to  light  touch.  Only  a  few  of  them  required 
strong  mechanical  stimuli.  These  comprised  80  per 
cent  of  the  total  of  63  thalamic  units  found,  whereas 
only  I  7  per  cent  responded  to  the  stronger  electrical 
stimulus  required  to  activate  saphenous  delta  fibers. 
Six-sevenths  of  these  required  stronger  stimuli  like 
squeezing,  pinching,  tapping  or  pricking  to  activate 


them.  Three  units  were  found  which  responded  to 
stimulation  of  saphenous  'C  fibers,  but  the  cutane- 
ous stimulus  which  would  fire  them  was  not  identi- 
fied. The  mean  latencies — knee  to  thalamus — were 
23,  47  and  630  msec.,  respectively,  for  the  three 
groups.  Some  representation  ipsilateral  as  well  as 
that  contralateral  to  the  stimulated  nerve  was  found 
in  the  monkey  thalamus.  There  was  no  anatomical 
segregation  among  the  different  types  of  unit  de- 
scribed and  there  was  likewise  a  huge  overlap  be- 
tween regions  for  face,  forelimb  and  hindlimb  both 
in  cat  and  monkey,  responses  from  the  face  even 
being  obtained  in  the  leg  area.  Upon  subtraction  of 
the  peripheral  conduction  time  from  the  total  la- 
tency one  obtained  a  central  conduction  time  in- 
cluding synaptic  dela\'  averaging  15  m  per  sec.  for 
the  alpha,  beta,  gamma  group,  7.9  m  per  sec.  for  the 
delta  groups  and  0.66  m  per  sec.  for  C  fibers. 

Dclgado's  (62)  monkeys  with  chronic  implanted 
electrodes  also  exhibited  behavior  suggesting  pain 
when  the  thalamic  nucleus  ventralis  posterior  was 
stimulated.  However,  electrical  stimulation  within 
the  thalamus  of  conscious  man  as  reported  by 
Talairach  et  al.  (260)  and  by  Hecaen  et  al.  (124)  did 
not  cause  actual  pain,  although  the  centrum  me- 
dianum, the  nucleus  ventralis  posteromedialis  and  the 
nucleus  medialis  dorsalis  were  the  presumed  sites  of 
stimulation  in  five  of  their  patients.  Nevertheless  the 
making  of  electrical  lesions  mainly  in  the  region  of 
the  centrum  medianum  caused  hypalgesia  to  anal- 
gesia over  varying  extents  of  the  contralateral  half 
of  head,  limbs  or  torso  along  with  reduction  or 
elimination  of  the  clinical  complaint  of  contralateral 
pain.  In  agreement  with  the  observations  that  the 
medial  lemniscus  terminates  close  to  this  area  there 
were  in  these  patients  also  varying  degrees  of  contra- 
lateral loss  of  touch,  position  and  vibratory  sen.se 
and  stereognosis. 

Following  thrombosis  of  the  thalamogeniculate 
artery  in  man  there  is  typically  an  extensive  de- 
struction of  the  posterior  part  of  the  lateral  nuclear 
mass  of  the  thalamus  which  contains  the  nuclei 
receiving  fibers  from  the  general  afferent  systems  and 
projects  to  the  postcentral  gyrus  of  the  cerebral 
cortex.  This  lesion  produces  among  other  signs  a 
transitory  complete  contralateral  hemianalgesia  as 
part  of  the  classical  thalamic  syndrome  of  Dejerine 
&  Roussy  (61).  This  sign  soon  gives  way  to  painful 
sensations  upon  noxious  stimulation;  later  these 
occur  upon  milder  stimulation  such  as  touch,  vibra- 
tion, pressure  or  sound;  and  finally  there  may  ap- 
pear a  state  of  spontaneous,  constant  or  paroxysmal 


492 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEITROPHVSIOLOGY    I 


pain  on  the  affected  side  which  no  longer  requires  a 
discrete  external  stimulus  for  its  appearance.  The 
unpleasant  sensations,  often  worse  than  those  after 
the  operation  of  mesencephalic  tractotomy  a)  are 
diffuse  and  peculiarly  disagreeable,  h}  come  on  only 
after  a  latent  period  of  i  or  more  sec,  c)  are  localized 
with  gross  errors,  (f)  may  appear  only  at  a  high 
threshold  and  i)  persist  after  the  stimulus  is  removed. 
It  is  not  yet  known  whether  permanent  hemianal- 
gesia  is  produced  by  a  sutliciently  massi\e  lesion  to 
destroy  all  of  the  presently  described  thalamic  nu- 
clei of  termination  of  somatic  and  visceral  afferent 
pathways.  From  many  of  the  published  descriptions 
one  cannot  l)e  certain  that  even  the  whole  nucleus 
ventralis  posterior  has  been  destroyed.  Walker's 
careful  report  illustrates  preservation  of  some  of 
the  nucleus  ventralis  posterolateralis  in  his  patient 
(279,  p.  81).  Nor  do  we  know  if  a  complete  lesion 
would  preclude  the  appearance  of  the  frightful  con- 
tralateral dysesthesias  and  pain  of  the  thalamic 
syndrome.  Now  that  many  more  nuclei  of  termina- 
tion for  spinothalamic  fibers  have  been  found  in 
man  by  Bowsher  it  .seems  even  more  likely  that  the 
thalamic  lesions  recorded  to  date  leave  intact  por- 
tions of  the  pain  pathways.  Their  relation  to  the 
distortion  of  pain  sensation  remains  to  be  elucidated. 


CEREBR.AL    HE.MISPHERES 

The  parts  of  the  cerebrum  known  to  be  concerned 
with  pain  have  been  established  by  a  variety  of  con- 
trived and  spontaneous  irritations  and  destructions. 

Stinndatidn 

Foerster  (77,  pp.  141  to  144),  one  of  the  early 
workers  to  explore  extensively  in  man  the  responses 
to  electrical  stimulation  of  the  cerebral  cortex,  found 
the  usual  response  from  the  postcentral  gyrus  or 
superior  parietal  lobule  to  consist  of  contralateral 
paresthesias,  occasionally  so  strong  as  to  be  painful. 
The  sensations  were  referred  to  a  comparativelv 
small  area  of  the  body  on  stimulus  to  the  postcentral 
gyrus,  whereas  from  the  less  excitable  superior 
parietal  lobule  a  response  when  obtained  was  re- 
ferred "more  or  less  to  the  entire  half  of  the  body." 
Although  the  majority  of  the  responses  are  referred 
to  or  near  the  body  surface,  visceral  pain  is  also 
represented  in  the  postcentral  gyrus.  'Cardiac  pain' 
and  severe  abdominal  pain  have  been  reported  when 
areas   for   the   upper   and    lower    trunk    respectively 


were  stimulated  (78,  p.  363).  The  response  inay  be 
complex,  especiallv  when  a  lesion  affects  the  stimu- 
lated area.  As  a  part  of  one  such  long  complicated 
response  to  stimulation  of  the  postcentral  gvrus, 
Krause  and  Schum  (78,  p.  363)  provoked  frightful 
pain  referred  ipsilaterally  to  an  upper  liinb. 

Penfield  and  his  associates  have  given  us  the  most 
complete  maps  of  cerebral  localization  based  on  the 
technique  of  cortical  stimulation.  Penfield  &  Boldrey 
(210)  found  that  such  stimulus  of  the  cerebral  surface 
rarely  causes  frank  pain,  in  fact  in  only  1  i  out  of  462 
responses.  But  nearly  half  of  the  reports  were  of 
'tinglins;'  or  'electricity,'  which  at  least  raises  the 
question  of  activity  in  pain  pathways.  The  great 
majority  of  the  points  on  the  superolateral  surface  of 
the  hemisphere  from  which  a  stimulus  elicited  sensa- 
tion are  in  the  postcentral  gyrus,  but  many  are  in 
the  precentral  gyrus,  and  a  few  lie  anterior  or  pos- 
terior to  these  two  gyri.  Penfield  &  Boldrey  obtained 
no  ipsilateral  .sensory  responses  but  the  reference  was 
at  times  bilateral  for  the  face,  tongue  and  eyes. 
Bilateral  pressure  sensations  encircling  the  torso 
were  noted  by  Foerster. 

In  the  subcortical  white  matter  the  limited  ex- 
plorations thus  far  carried  out  have  yielded  to 
Hecaen  et  al.  (124)  an  area  in  the  parietal  lobe  deep 
to  the  gyrus  cinguli  in  which  weak  electrical  stimuli 
provoked  violent  localized  lightning-like  pain. 

Patients  with  severe  pain  elsewhere  in  the  body, 
upon  cortical  stimulation,  seem  especially  prone  to 
feel  pain  often  akin  to  the  clinical  complaint.  Thus 
Horrax  (129)  elicited  pain  upon  strong  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  postcentral  gyrus  in  three  of  four 
patients  suffering  from  painful  states.  Erickson  et  al. 
(74)  recorded  similar  results  in  three  of  five  patients 
afflicted  with  either  a  painful  phantom  limb  or  the 
syndrome  of  thalamic  pain.  Both  patients  with  pain 
probleins  described  by  White  &  Sweet  (296,  pp.  334 
to  337  and  p.  413)  had  pain  upon  stimulation  of  the 
postcentral  gyrus,  and  in  one  case  even  more  severe 
pain  occurred  upon  stimulation  of  the  precentral 
gyrus.  This  patient's  spontaneous  pain  in  each 
phantom  finger  was  stopped  dramatically  as  the 
appropriate  area  of  the  postcentral  gyrus  was  in- 
jected subpially  with  procaine.  Lewin  &  Phillips 
(169)  reproduced  preoperative  pain — either  a  part 
of  an  epileptic  seizure  or  a  painful  phantom — upon 
stimulation  of  the  postcentral  gvrus  in  three  of  three 
patients  and  secured  relief  by  removal  of  this  area 
of  the  cortex. 

Cortical  and  subcortical  lesions  have  also  pro- 
vided irritative  foci  giving  rise  to  pain,  a)  as  an  aura 


PAIN 


493 


to  or  part  of  a  focal  seizure;  A)  as  a  more  continuous 
pain;  or  c)  as  a  dysesthesia  appearing  only  when  the 
surface  of  the  body  was  stimulated.  Michelsen  (189) 
reviews  earlier  reports  and  adds  five  new  cases  of 
his  own.  Paroxysmal  abdominal  pain  as  a  form  of 
epilepsy  has  now  been  reported  in  several  series  of 
patients.  O'Brien  &  Goldensohn  (201)  summarize 
the  earlier  work  and  add  their  own  observations 
which  indicate  that  in  at  least  some  of  these  patients 
organic  cerebral  lesions  cause  attacks  of  pain  pri- 
marily referred  to  the  abdomen,  whereas  in  other 
patients  the  pain  is  secondary  to  abnormal  gastroin- 
testinal motility. 

Lesions 

Destruction  of  appropriate  cortex  and  subcortical 
white  matter  may  also  cause  hypalgesia,  rarely 
analgesia.  One  of  the  early  reports  implicating  the 
cerebral  cortex  with  pain  perception  is  Dejerine  & 
Mouzon's  (60)  account  in  191 5  of  a  war  casualty 
whose  small  cortical  wound  produced  loss  of  pain 
sensibility  in  the  contralateral  arm.  Kleist  (14J,  pp. 
426  to  428)  collected  24  patients  wounded  in  World 
War  I  in  whom  a  localized  parietal  lesion  brought 
about  disturbances  mainly  in  pain  and  thermal 
senses.  In  eight  of  these,  hypalgesia  was  the  only 
sensory  loss.  He  noted  analgesia  changing  dining 
convalescence  at  times  to  an  abnormally  increased 
appreciation  of  stimuli,  a  hyperpathia.  He  ventured 
without  confirmation  post-mortem  a  precise  place- 
ment of  the  cortical  area  for  pain  and  temperature 
sensations  in  the  posterior  bank  of  the  central  fis- 
sure— in  Brodmann's  narrow  fields  3a  and  3b. 
Russell  (233)  after  a  study  of  men  wounded  in 
World  War  H  reached  essentially  identical  conclu- 
sions on  all  of  the  above  .scores.  Davison  &  Schick 
(57)  have  also  described  two  patients  with  hyper- 
pathia combined  with  hypalgesia  in  whom  autopsy 
revealed  only  cortical  and  subcortical  lesions,  com- 
pletely sparing  the  thalamus. 

Foerster  (78,  p.  146)  pointed  out  that  the  sub- 
normality  in  pain  sensation  after  removal  of  the  post- 
central gyral  representation  for  a  limb  soon  returns 
virtually  to  normal;  but  subtle  changes  such  as  a 
reduction  in  the  number  of  pain  points  or  an  increase 
in  their  threshold  persisted  for  years  in  seven  patients 
studied  by  Kroll  (147).  As  early  as  1909  Horsley 
(130)  had  noted  that  even  after  removal  of  the 
'whole  arm  centre'  in  both  precentral  and  postcen- 
tral gyri  the  pain  sensation  was  'notably  diminished' 
though    not    abolished.    Marshall    (184)    ably    sum- 


marizes the  earlier  work  and  adds  studies  on  12  more 
war-injured  patients  examined  5  to  34  years  after- 
ward. In  some  area  contralateral  to  a  shallow  cere- 
bral wound,  all  experienced  slight  or  no  pain  when 
tested  both  with  a  heavy  pin  jab  and  by  injection  of 
0.2  cc  of  6  per  cent  sodium  chloride  into  the  muscles 
in  an  effort  to  provoke  deep  pain.  He  showed  clearly 
the  possibility  of  protracted  focal  severe  disturbance 
of  appreciation  of  pain  from  such  lesions.  Both  he 
and  Russell  have  commented  on  the  anomalous 
situation  that  an  extensive  cortical  injury  may  leave 
pain  sensibilit\-  intact,  whereas  a  small  cortical  wound 
in  part  of  the  same  area  in  another  patient  mav  pro- 
duce hypalgesia. 

That  a  massive  area  of  the  cerebral  cortex  may  in 
some  way  be  associated  with  irritating  sensation  was 
shown  by  Dusser  de  Barenne  and  his  collaborators 
(70)  in  experiments  in  lower  primates.  For  example 
when  they  applied  strychnine  locally  over  a  few 
square  millimeters  of  the  cortex  anywhere  over  about 
the  posterior  half  of  the  frontal  lobe  or  the  anterior 
three-quarters  of  the  parietal  lobe  of  the  chimpan- 
zee, they  set  up  a  diffuse  irritation  in  face,  arms  or 
legs,  depending  on  the  area  of  cortex  to  which  the 
drug  was  applied.  The  animal  licked  or  scratched 
the  skin  of  the  zones  concerned  for  about  30  min. 
more  vigorously  contralateral  than  ipsilateral  to  the 
side  of  the  placement  of  the  drug.  The  electrocortico- 
gram  showed  "strychnine  spikes'  within  this  extensive 
sensory  region,  according  to  Bailey  et  al.  (12). 

The  ipsilateral  cerebral  representation  of  pain 
intimated  lay  these  studies  is  further  suggested  bv 
the.se  facts.  Total  hemispherectomy  in  man  does  not 
produce  complete  contralateral  analgesia,  but  such  a 
degree  of  cortical  removal  in  the  macaque  and 
chimpanzee  provokes  almost  complete  degeneration 
in  every  thalamic  nucleus  except  those  in  the  medul- 
lary laminae  which  do  not  project  to  the  cerebral 
cortex  (279).  This  leaves  the  ipsilateral  thalamus  and 
cerebral  cortex  as  the  most  likely  sites  mediating 
pain  perception  following  hemispherectom\-.  Some 
individuals,  e.g.  Evans'  patient  reported  by  Walker 
(279),  show  but  little  disturbance  of  appreciation  of 
pinprick  anywhere  except  for  some  delay  in  response, 
whereas  in  two  patients  of  Dandy  (52)  there  was 
said  to  be  loss  of  all  contralateral  cutaneous  sensation 
below  the  face  with  a  varying  but  lesser  lo.ss  in  the 
face.  Gardner  et  al.  (88)  have  recently  given  a  resume 
of  the  findings  in  their  own  and  the  earlier  reported 
cases.  They  found  a  striking  and  constant  retention 
of  all  modalities  of  sensation  in  the  trigeminal  area 
both  in   patients  whose  operation  was  for  infantile 


494 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    1 


hemiplegia  and  in  those  who  had  tumors.  This  ex- 
tensive bilateral  cortical  representation  for  facial 
sensation  including  pain  is  consonant  with  the  well- 
known  similar  motor  representation.  On  the  average 
the  tumor  patients  showed  the  greater  deficit,  the 
lesion  having  been  present  a  shorter  time  in  a  more 
adult  brain.  The  contralateral  parts  showed  an  im- 
pairment of  appreciation  and  localization  of  a  sharp 
point,  which  increased  progres><i\ely  in  the  following 
sequence:  face,  trunk,  thigh,  upper  arm,  leg,  fore- 
arm, foot  and  hand.  A  delay  in  the  appreciation  of 
all  stimuli  on  the  paralyzed  side  was  short  in  the 
trigeminal  area  and  longest  in  the  distal  parts  of 
the  limbs.  Pain  elicited  from  the  abnormal  side  was 
more  disagreeable  than  that  from  the  normal  side. 
Cold  and  hot  stimuli  were  painful  and  could  not  be 
differentiated.  When  a  pin  was  applied  simultane- 
ously to  similar  sites  on  the  two  sides,  there  was 
consistent  extinction  only  below  the  elbow  and  knee 
on  the  paretic  side. 

Evoked  Potentials 

The  somatic  sensory  areas  I  and  II  of  the  cerebral 
cortex  of  many  mammals  including  monkeys  have 
been  outlined  on  the  Ijasis  of  cortical  electrical 
potentials  evoked  by  tactile  stimuli  to  the  body 
surface  (311).  There  has  i^een  very  little  work  to 
determine  in  animals  the  cerebral  representation  for 
painful  stimuli;  but  in  continuation  of  our  assump- 
tion that  'A'  gamma-delta  impulses  may  be  asso- 
ciated with  pain,  we  shall  summarize  the  work  of 
Amassian  (5,  6)  on  the  cortical  respon.ses  evoked 
from  such  fibers  in  the  splanchnic  nerves  of  car- 
nivores. From  tiny  areas  on  the  cortex  at  the  junction 
of  leg  and  arm  representation  in  both  sen.sory  areas 
I  and  II  in  the  dog  and  cat,  he  found  brief  initially 
surface-positive  waves.  These  were  obtained  from 
both  sensory  areas  contralaterally  and  from  the 
ipsilateral  area  II  in  the  cat  when  only  splanchnic 
'A'  beta  fibers  were  excited,  as  shown  in  figure  11. 
At  much  higher  voltage,  when  'A'  gamma-delta 
activation  was  also  visible  in  the  record  from  the 
splanchnic  nerve,  a  small  deflection  appeared  on  the 
returning  limb  of  the  primary  response  in  area  I 
as  the  only  early  cortical  evidence  of  presumed 
activity  of  pain  fibers.  There  was  no  change  in  the 
primary  response  from  area  II.  Activation  of  the 
gamma-delta  fibers  also  evoked  a  late  secondary 
response  generalized  over  the  cortex.  In  the  monkey, 
Ruch  et  al.  (232)  were  able  to  find  a  splanchnic 
representation  only  in  cortical  area  I. 


0.13v. 


3.8  V. 


FIG.  1 1 .  Splanchnic  A  gamma-delta  fibers  and  cerebral 
evoked  potentials.  Stimulating  electrodes  on  splanchnic  nerve 
distally.  Upper  records  obtained  with  stimulus  0.13  v.;  there  is  a 
maximal  primary  response  from  cortical  area  1  Qupper  /efl~)  with 
no  A  gamma-delta  discharge  on  the  neurogram  of  the  sym- 
pathetic trunk  O'Pper  right^.  Lower  records  obtained  with  stim- 
ulus 3.8  V.  and  pulse  duration  i  msec,  the  A  gamma-delta 
group  is  active  (^second  wave,  lower  right'),  but  the  only  cortical 
correlate  therewith  is  a  small  deflection  on  the  returning  limb 
of  the  primary  response  (lower  lejl).  [From  Amassian  (5).] 


A  somewhat  similar  type  of  study  has  been  carried 
out  in  the  cat  by  Mountcastle  et  al.  (196)  working 
with  nerves  to  muscle.  They  monitored  oscilloscopi- 
cally  the  ventral  root  instead  of  the  stimulated 
exposed  nerve  and  took  as  evidence  of  excitation  of 
the  Group  III  fibers,  i.e.  "the  delta  pile,'  the  ap- 
pearance of  a  late  polysynaptic  spinal  cord  reflex  in 
the  ventral  roots.  Only  at  stimuli  .sufficiently  intense 
to  excite  these  fibers  did  they  evoke  potentials  in 
the  contralateral  cerebral  cortex.  Responses  were 
seen  in  both  somatic  areas  I  and  II;  they  were  of 
higher  amplitude  in  area  II.  Their  latency  at  18  to 
19  msec,  was  about  twice  that  of  cortical  potentials 
seen  upon  stimulation  of  adjacent  cutaneous  nerves. 
Because  the  small  myelinated  fibers  in  these  muscle 
nerves  are  from  i  to  8  /x  in  diameter  and  conduct 
up  to  40  m  per  sec,  it  was  thought  that  their  im- 
pulses might  include  some  of  nociceptive  character. 

Second  Sensory  Area  in  Man 

Penfield  &  Rasmussen  (211)  have  shown  in  man 
that  sensation  can  be  evoked  from  the  secondary 
sensory  area  at  the  lowest  part  of  the  postcentral 
gyrus  extending  into  the  superior  lip  of  the  Sylvian 
fissure   to   include   part   of  the   parietal   operculum. 


PAIN 


495 


Only  a  few  of  the  reported  sensations  ha\e  been  de- 
scribed as  'pricking.'  However,  Biemond  (25)  has 
described  a  remarkable  case  in  which  a  complex  of 
small  confluent  foci  of  softening  was  found  in  the 
cortex  and  white  matter  of  the  right  parietal  opercu- 
lum (see  fig.  1 2)  and  in  the  cortex  of  the  insula.  This 
lesion  had  been  as.sociated  with  severe  hypalgesia 
over  the  entire  left  half  of  the  body,  as  well  as  with 
a  constant  deep  'drilling'  pain  throughout  this  area 
worsened  by  any  local  stimulus.  The  senses  of  touch, 
proprioception,  attitude,  stereognosis,  vibration, 
graphesthesia  and  discrimination  were  all  intact !  In 
figure  12  one  sees  also  retrograde  degeneration  of  a 
fiber  bundle  passing  into  the  posteroventral  nucleus 
of  the  thalamus  where  a  marked  cellular  loss  had 
occurred.  This  loss  was  worse  in  the  caudal  portion 
of  the  nucleus  in  which  the  spinothalamic  fibers 
principally    terminate.    He    also    reports    two    other 


less  striking  but  similar  patients  in  whom  the  findings 
in  life  and  at  autopsy  also  suggest  that  the  second 
sensory  area  is  related  to  the  'conscious  pain  sensa- 
tion.' 

A  review  of  the  earlier  literature  discloses  that  the 
lesion  in  Davison  &  Schick's  (57)  case  10  was  largely 
in  the  second  sensory  area  but  with  involvement  also 
of  the  superior  temporal  and  insular  cortex.  The 
sensory  findings,  similar  regarding  pain  to  those  in 
Biemond's  (25)  case  i,  included  spontaneous  and 
touch-evoked  pains — although  in  this  patient  touch, 
vibration  and  stereognosis  were  impaired  also.  This 
case  report,  made  before  Adrian  had  described  the 
second  sensory  area,  may  in  retrospect  be  taken  as 
confirmation  of  such  an  area  in  man  and  as  adding 
evidence  that  it  is  especially  concerned  with  the 
sense  of  pain. 


NUCLEUS     LATERALIS 
THALAMI 

NUCLEUS     MEDIALIS 
THALAMI 

NUCLEUS    CENTRUM 
MEDIANUM 

NUCLEUS    VENTRALIS 

POSTEROMEDIALIS      AND 

POSTEROLATERALIS 


_____CORPUS    GENICULATUM 
MEDIALE 


CORPUS    GENICULATUM 
LATERALS 


FIG.  12.  Pain  and  the  second  sensory  area  in  man.  Fine  diagonal  hatching:  areas  of  softening  in  the 
parietal  operculum  and  cortex  of  insula;  these  extended  in  varying  degree  from  (he  corona]  plane 
of  the  anterior  commissure  in  front  to  that  of  the  lateral  geniculate  body  behind  Heavy  diagonal 
hatching:  retrograde  degeneration  of  fiber  bundle  as  seen  in  Weigert-Pal  stain  for  myelin,  passing 
into  posteroventral  nucleus  of  thalamus  via  posterior  part  of  internal  capsule.  Heavy  dots:  marked 
cellular  loss  in  nucleus  ventralis  posteromedialis  and  posterolateralis,  especially  in  caudal  portion 
01    nucleus.    [Based   on    data   from   Biemond    (25).] 


496 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Remtton  to  Pain 

The  complexities  of  the  cerebral  mechanisms  re- 
sponsible finalK  for  normal  appreciation  of  and  reac- 
tion to  pain  are  at  present  lara;ely  beyond  our 
knowledge. 

Using  the  radiant  heat  method  of  Hardy  et  ai 
(115),  Chapman  (39)  studied  thresholds  to  'pain 
perception,'  i.e.  the  subjective  end  point  signalled  by 
the  patient;  and  to  'pain  reaction,'  the  first  objective 
evidence  of  withdrawal,  such  as  wincing,  seen  by  the 
examiner.  He  found  that  a  group  of  psychoneurotic 
patients,  while  presenting  abnormal  pain  perception, 
showed  an  abnormally  low  threshold  for  pain  reaction. 

INDIFFERENCE  TO  p.AiN.  The  Contrary  state,  indif- 
ference to  pain,  has  been  seen  temporarily  in  periods 
of  severe  emotional  stress  or  in  hypnosis,  or  over  longer 
periods  of  time  in  hysteria,  psychosis  and  posten- 
cephalitic states,  and  in  mental  defectives.  In  the 
latter  two  groups  no  eflTort  has  been  made  to  correlate 
any  particular  lesions  of  the  brain  with  this  s\ mptom. 
An  extraordinary  and  rare  phenomenon,  described 
as  'a  congenital  insensiti\ity  to  pain'  of  many  types, 
may  occur  in  people  otherwise  apparently  nearly 
normal.  A  detailed  perusal  of  the  case  reports  is  re- 
quired to  appreciate  the  severity  and  \ariety  of  the 
injuries  and  noxious  stimuli  which  such  indi\iduals 
have  repeatedly  sustained  without  pain  (32,  51,  59, 
81,  136,  148,  224,  236).  Seven  other  case  reports 
are  cited  by  Madonick  (182),  although  his  own  case, 
I  think,  belongs  in  the  mental  defective  group  men- 
tioned above.  A  feature  common  to  nearlv  all  of  these 
indi\iduals  has  been  their  ability  to  distinguish  with 
fair  or  great  accuracy  between  the  point  and  head  of 
a  pin  or  between  slight  differences  of  temperature. 
Yet  they  are  indifferent  to  violent  jaijs  and  extremes 
of  temperature  and  their  utter  lack  of  suffering  is 
their  striking  characteristic.  Boyd  &  Nie's  (32)  phrase 
"congenital  universal  indifference  to  pain"  more 
clearly  indicates  the  person's  beha\ior  and  that 
the  abnormality  is  central  rather  than  peripheral.  A 
number  of  them  do  experience  discomfort  upon  cu- 
taneous electrical  stimulation  at  high  levels,  morbid 
distension  of  viscera,  or  other  extreme  noxa.  The 
brain  of  no  such  person  has  yet  been  studied  histo- 
logically, but  the  occurrence  of  various  types  of 
seizures  or  minor  mental  defect  in  several  of  them 
intimate  that  an  organic  lesion  is  present. 

PAIN  ASYMBOLIA.  A  State  with  slight  similarity  to  the 
foregoing  has  been  described  by  Schilder  &  Stengel 


(239)  as  'pain  asymbolia,'  the  situation  in  which  there 
is  "no  analgesia  in  the  common  sen.se,  but  the  psvchic 
reaction  to  the  sensation  is  absent."  They  (240)  have 
observed  this  symptom  in  10  patients  with  acquired 
organic  cerebral  disease  and  ha\e  implicated  the 
anterior  part  of  the  lower  part  of  the  dominant 
parietal  lobe.  Autopsies  on  three  of  the  patients 
showed  \'arious  lesions  of  which  the\-  thought  the 
common  denominator  was  involvement  of  the  supra- 
marginal  gyrus.  However  in  these  patients  there  was 
a  concomitant  sensory  aphasia  which  made  appraisal 
difficult,  and  at  least  in  soine  of  them  there  was  a 
"dulling  of  the  appreciation  of  pain"  as  well  as  an 
insufficient  pain  reaction.  Moreover  this  dullness  ex- 
tended to  a  lack  of  concern  over  threatening  gestures 
made  toward  the  patient,  intimating  a  general  dis- 
turbance of  the  capacity  to  appreciate  danger. 

Rubins  &  Friedman  (230)  have  contrif)uted  four 
more  patients  in  whom  this  general  clinical  picture 
was  present  and  in  two  of  whom  operati\e  findings 
placed  the  lesion  mainly  in  the  dominant  inferior 
parietal  region.  Although  these  patients  recognized 
a  pin  as  sharp,  they  did  not  withdraw  from  either 
painful  stimuli  or  threatening  gestures.  But  thev  also 
showed  mild  perceptive  and  more  se\ere  amnestic 
aphasia,  right-left  disorientation,  inaljility  to  repro- 
duce postural  attitudes  in  space,  Gerstmann's  svn- 
dromc  and  idiokinetic  apraxia.  Hence,  the  'asymbolia 
for  pain'  is  b\-  no  means  the  isolated  phenomenon 
seen  in  the  syndrome  of  congenital  indifference 
to  pain.  Hecaen  &  de  Ajuriaguerra  ('''3).  adding  a 
case  report,  note  that  in  a  number  of  other  recorded 
patients  as  well  as  theirs  the  lesion  extended  into  the 
posterior  inferior  part  of  the  frontal  lobe. 

.\n  e\-en  greater  variety  of  locus  of  lesion  was  seen 
by  VVeinstein  et  al.  (290)  in  15  patients  with  pain 
asymbolia  who  did  not  have  any  aphasia.  Such  a 
group  was  .selected  for  a  special  study  of  personalit\-, 
and  many  of  the  patients  had  lesions  in  the  non- 
dominant  hemisphere.  Their  heedlessness  of  noxious 
stimulation  was  often  accompanied  b\  inattention  to 
disabled  parts,  by  muteness,  by  hypokinesia  or  by 
depression.  The  authors  considered  all  of  these  symp- 
toms as  to  some  extent  an  implicit  denial  of  illness, 
perhaps  related  to  actual  \erbal  denial  of  illness  or 
anosognosia.  They  considered  such  behavior  related 
more  to  the  personality  background  of  the  patient 
than  to  any  specific  lesion  in  the  brain,  and  thought 
the  premorbid  personality  of  patients  with  'pain 
asymbolia'  was  characterized  by  the  habitual  use  of 
withdrawal  and  axoidance  in  stressful  situations.  It 
is  apparent  that  the  attribution  of  a  depres.sed  reac- 


PAIN 


497 


tion  to  all  noxa  to  a  sharply  focal  cerebral  lesion  has 
dubious  validitv. 


REACTIONS  AFTER  OPERATIONS  ON   FRONTAL  LOBES. 

The  modifications  in  the  reaction  of  the  individual 
to  painful  or  distressing  states  provoked  by  removal  of 
cortex  or  division  of  white  fibers  in  the  anterior  two- 
thirds  of  the  frontal  lobe  remain  to  be  considered. 
Such  lesions  in  an  otherwise  normal  brain  diminish 
the  general  reaction  to  constant  pain  of  organic  cause, 
such  as  advancing  cancer,  as  well  as  the  reaction  to 
such  psychological  suff"ering  as  may  be  occasioned  by 
the  knowledge  of  impending  death,  an  obsessive  com- 
pulsive psychoneurosis  or  psychotic  agitated  depres- 
sion. But  the  price  paid  for  such  relief  includes  in- 
ability to  experience  keen  pleasure  as  well,  i.e.  there 
is  a  flattening  of  all  affect  and  the  de\elopment  of  a 
more  or  less  apathetic  state.  In  addition  a  wide  variety 
of  evidences  of  mental  deficit  may  appear.  The 
greater  the  area  of  frontal  lobe  removed  or  deprived 
of  its  normal  connections  by  division  of  white  matter, 
the  greater  the  deficit.  When  most  of  the  frontal  white 
matter  of  a  normally  functioning  human  brain  is 
transected  bilaterally  in  the  coronal  plane  just  an- 
terior to  the  lateral  ventricles,  there  is  often  a  serious 
disturbance  of  intellect  and  personality,  as  described 
by  Rylander  (234),  Freeman  &  Watts  (84,  pp.  360 
to  374),  and  Krayenbiihl  &  Stoll  (146).  In  an  oc- 
casional patient  these  defects  are  mild  enough  to 
permit  the  indi\idual  to  return  to  his  work  and 
retain  for  years  a  useful  degree  of  pain  relief  (84,  pp. 
367  to  368;  205,  pp.  452  to  453). 

In  an  effort  to  secure  a  fruitful  result  with  re.spect 
to  pain  but  to  preserve  the  personalit\,  small  lesions 
have  been  made.  A  total  division  of  the  white  fibers 
on  one  side  only,  according  to  Koskoff  c/  al.  (145)  and 
Scarff  (235),  produces  a  lesser  deficit  from  which 
there  is  usually  much  recovery,  unfortunately  accom- 
panied pari  passu  by  return  of  pain.  No  significant 
difference  in  result  re  pain  has  been  noted  between 
division  of  fibers  contralateral  or  ipsilateral  to  one- 
sided pain  or  between  operations  in  the  dominant  or 
nondominant  hemisphere.  Bilateral  inferior  quadrant, 
bilateral  medial  or  inferomedial  lesions (i  1 1),  removal 
of  various  small  portions  of  the  frontal  lobes 
bilaterally,  i.e.  topectomy  (216)  or  undercutting  of 
various  parts  of  the  frontal  cortex  (244)  have  all  been 
performed.  Such  patients  have  as  yet  been  less  thor- 
oughly studied  in  relation  to  the  correlation  between 
locus  of  lesion  and  relief  of  pain,  but  the  general 
pattern  is  similar  in  all.  Contrary  to  the  situation  in 


pain  asymbolia,  the  lobotomized  patient's  reaction 
to  individual  noxious  stimuli  is,  if  anything,  increased. 
He  jumps  at  pinpricks  and  needle  punctures  and 
responds  in  the  Hardy-Wolff-Goodell  pain-threshold 
apparatus  by  wincing  and  pulling  his  hand  away  at 
a  lesser  stimulus  after  operation  than  before  it,  ac- 
cording to  Chapman  et  al.  (41).  The  general  experi- 
ence amply  confirms  Freeman  &  Watts'  observations 
(84,  pp.  371  and  372)  that  such  events  as  rectal  dila- 
tation or  childbirth  are  distressing  to  lobotomy  pa- 
tients. Moreover,  following  lobotomy  when  questioned 
about  their  preoperative  pain  they  are  likely  to  state 
that  it  is  'just  as  bad  as  ever'  or  "terrible."  Yet  they 
ha\e  few  or  no  spontaneous  complaints  of  pain,  ask 
for  little  or  no  medication  even  if  narcotic  addiction 
appeared  to  be  a  problem  before  operation  and  are 
far  less  miserable  even  when  mentation  is  almost 
normal.  Patients  with  significant  mental  deficits  may 
deny  pain  on  direct  questioning  or  even  forget  about 
the  illness  which  is  causing  the  pain.  LeBeau  (159, 
pp.  134  to  135  and  pp.  226  to  290}  and  White  & 
Sweet  (296,  pp.  287  to  333)  summarize  earlier  reports 
and  give  accounts  of  their  own  experiences. 

The  behavior  of  the  patients  suggests  that  per- 
sistence either  of  noxious  physical  stimuli  or  dis- 
turbing thoughts  sets  off  in  the  normal  frontal  lobes 
a  potentiating  mechanism  which  becomes  a  major 
factor  in  the  total  suffering  of  the  person.  That  this 
mechanism  may  be  to  some  extent  specific  to  the 
frontal  lobes  is  illustrated  by  the  failure  of  bilateral 
anterior  temporal  lobectomy  to  modify  the  reactions 
to  pain  (296,  p.  319). 

That  the  mechanism  may  involve  the  diffuse 
thalamic  projection  system  of  Morison  &  Dempsey 
(191)  is  suggested  by  the  following  experiments.  The 
thalamic  nuclei  of  the  macaque  monkey  giving  rise 
to  this  projection  system  are  the  .same  ones  which 
recei\'e  afferent  impulses  of  .somatic  and  visceral  origin 
from  the  reticular  activating  system  lying  in  the 
medial  brain  stem  (85,  254).  These  impulses  are  dis- 
tributed in  certain  thalamic  association  nuclei  mainly 
to  the  frontal  lobe  anterior  to  areas  4  and  6.  Magoun 
and  associates  have  suggested  that  it  is  disturbance 
of  the  diffuse  thalamic  projection  system  which 
diminishes  the  aff^ective  component  of  sensorv  per- 
ception and  deprives  pain  of  its  unpleasantness,  the 
characteristic  state  following  a  frontal  lobotomy, 
cortical  undercutting  or  corticectomy.  Since  the 
dorsomedial  nucleus  of  the  thalamus  is  one  of  the 
main  association  nuclei  for  the  diffuse  thalamic 
projection  system  this  explanation  would  also  account 
for  the  similar  condition  following  operative  destruc- 


498 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


lion  of  this  nucleus.  Thus  Orchinik  ti  al.  (204),  work- 
ing with  patients  of  Spiegel  &  VVycis  in  whom  dorso- 
medial  thalamotomy  had  been  done,  found  reduced 
fearfulncss  of  emotionally  charged  situations  of  many 
types.  Yet  there  were  "no  changes  in  intellectual 
functioning,  as  measured  by  a  standardized  test." 

Conclusion 

Despite  all  of  the  foregoing  data  we  are  still  unable 
to  say  what  level  of  the  brain  must  be  attained  or  what 
constellation  of  nuclei  and  fibers  must  be  active  if 
pain  is  to  be  perceived.  And  not  only  do  we  not  know 
the  mechanisms  involved  in  conversion  of  awareness 
of  pain  to  the  more  grievous  state  of  real  suffering, 
we  are  uncertain  as  to  the  site  and  e.xtent  of  lesion 
required  to  preclude  the  appearance  of  suffering. 


ENDOCRINES    .AND    P.AIN 

The  recent  introduction  of  total  hypophysectomy 
in  man  as  a  palliative  treatment  for  advanced  cancer 
of  the  breast  has  provided  an  incidental  and  imex- 
pectedly  great  reduction  in  or  abolition  of  the  pain 
in  many  of  the  patients  so  afflicted.  Luft  &  Olivecrona 
(180)  saw  these  favorable  effects  in  19  of  24  women 
in  their  series,  and  B.  S.  Ray  has  stated  that  his 
results  are  similar.  The  relief  of  pain  occurs  promptly 
after  operation  and  is  not  due  to  cortisone  since  it 
continues  when  the  drug  is  stopped.  It  has  not  been 
correlated  with  subsidence  of  tumor  and  has  oc- 
curred both  in  patients  who  did  and  in  others  who  did 
not  give  objective  evidence  of  remission,  as  well  as  in 
patients  who  had  either  subtotal  or  complete  hy- 
pophvsectomy.  These  obserx^ations  open  for  considera- 
tion the  po.ssibility  that  endocrine  potentiation  of 
nervous  function  is  implicated  in  the  full  develop- 
ment of  pain. 


ITCHING    AND   TICKLING 

In  addition  to  the  various  sensations  under  the 
heading  of  pain  discussed  already  there  remain  for 
consideration  'itch'  and  'tickle.'  .Although  the  decisi\e 
feature  of  the  stimulus  which  will  lead  it  to  provoke 
an  itch  is  unclear,  the  nature  of  the  sensation,  i.e.  the 
desire  to  scratch,  is  universally  understood.  An 
abundance  of  evidence  now  exists  to  indicate  that 
itching  is  closely  related  to  cutaneous  pain.  In  a 
number  of  disorders  involving  a  loss  of  pain  with 


presersation  of  touch  and  proprioception,  the  capacity 
to  itch  in  the  analgesic  zone  was  long  ago  shown  to  be 
lost  (4,  266).  This  was  fully  confirmed  in  a  recent 
study  by  Arieff  ?/  al.  (7)  i)oth  in  patients  with  radicular 
and  in  those  with  cord  lesions  causing  the  dissociated 
decrease  or  loss  in  appreciation  of  a  pinprick  as  pain- 
ful. Itch  disappears  and  reappears  along  with  pain 
perception  in  the  reversible  states  of  asphyxial  nerve 
i^lock  (174,  175)  and  of  local  anesthesia  (227,  263). 
Rothman  (227)  has  also  noted  that  the  converse 
is  true,  i.e.  that  sensitivity  to  pain  and  itching  may  be 
preserved  in  patients  in  whom  there  were  zones  of 
complete  tactile  anesthesia.  He  has  also  reported 
itching  independent  of  the  sense  of  temperature. 
Zotterman  (315)  interpreted  .some  of  his  animal 
experimental  results  to  indicate  that  peripheral  af- 
ferent impul.ses  mediating  itching  traverse  C  fibers, 
but  the  crucial  proof  was  lacking,  namely  that  the  an- 
imal tended  to  scratch  a  zone  from  which  C  fiber  po- 
tentials were  arising. 

As  one  moves  into  the  central  nervous  svstem  the 
correlation  of  itching  with  pain  continues;  it  may  be 
followed  first  with  the  central  portion  of  the  pain 
fibers  of  the  primary  afferent  neuron.  Thus  operative 
di\ision  of  the  descending  trigeminal  tract  in  the 
medulla  oblongata  \ielded  complete  trigeminal 
analgesia  and  thermanesthesia  in  a  patient  who  had 
before  operation  such  extreme  itching  that  he 
scratched  out  all  the  hair  in  the  left  anterior  quadrant 
of  his  scalp.  Postoperatively  the  itching  stopped  and 
the  hair  grew  back  (296,  pp.  459  and  512).  Likewise 
division  of  pain  fibers  at  the  secondary  afferent  neuron 
by  section  of  the  anterior  quadrant  of  the  cord  has 
stopped  even  itching  of  pathologically  .severe  origin 
and  intensity.  This  was  first  noted  in  Sicard  & 
Robineau's  patient  (^4^)  and  in  Banzet's  (14)  case 
21,  each  with  bilateral  kraurosis  vulvae.  Further 
examples  are  mentioned  by  White  &  Sweet  (296, 
p.  261).  Bickford  C'^4)  produced  itching  by  puncturing 
histamine  solutions  into  the  normal  skin;  a  protracted 
itch  followed  the  use  of  a  1:15  dilution.  In  five  pa- 
tients, including  one  with  a  cordotomy  whose  spinal 
lesion  caused  loss  of  pain  to  pin  while  touch  was  pre- 
served, he  could  not  evoke  itching  from  the  analgesic 
skin.  Hyndman  &  W'olkin  (133)  were  likewise  imable 
to  provoke  itching  by  application  of  itch  powder 
(from  Miuuna  pruriens)  to  the  analgesic  areas  after 
cordotomy.  Control  areas  of  normal  sensation  did  itch. 

There  is  one  discordant  observation  by  Taylor 
(261).  His  patient  with  generalized  bilateral  itching 
continued  to  have  this  svmptom  in  the  analgesic  zone 
following  unilateral  bulbar  spinothalamic  tractotomy. 


499 


But  the  itching  postoperatively  seemed  'deeper  in'  on 
the  side  which  no  longer  felt  a  pin  as  sharp.  No  men- 
tion was  made  as  to  whether  or  not  pain  from  deeper 
structures  was  stopped  by  this  operation.  There  is 
also  the  possibility  that  the  itching  in  this  patient 
was  of  central  origin  since  he  felt  it  everywhere  and 
had  no  primary  skin  disease.  Intense  itching  appears 
to  have  been  provoked  by  stimulus  to  the  brain  in 
cats  upon  intracisternal  injections  of  morphine, 
physostigmine,  pilocarpine  or  acetylcholine  Ci44)> 
and  upon  intraventricular  injection  of  diisopropyl- 
fluorophosphate  (DFP)  or  physostigmine  (76).  A 
centrally  induced  itch  probably  does  not  require 
intact  spinothalamic  pathways  to  achieve  conscious 
recognition. 

Investigations  of  the  sense  of  tickle  suffer  from  the 
difficulty  one  has  in  describing  the  sensation.  For 
Pritchard  (217)  and  Foerster  (78)  it  is  'itching  of  the 
weakest  intensity'  and  corresponding  to  this  concept 
Rothman  (228)  describes  the  after-sensation  following 
light  strokes,  firm  strokes  and  burning  stimuli  on  the 
skin  as  tickle,  itch  and  burning  pain,  respectively — all 
mediated  by  'C  fibers,  he  says.  These  speculations 
lack  factual  support.  Foerster  &  Gagel  (80)  and  Bick- 
ford  (24),  pursuing  the  subject  into  the  spinal  cord, 
found  tickle  sensation  ab.sent  in  the  analgesic  area 
after  cordotomy;  but  patients  of  Feet  (208),  Hyndman 
&  VVolkin  (133)  and  White  &  Sweet  (296,  p.  261) 
said  they  could  still  be  tickled  in  such  areas.  It  .scarcely 
seems  worth  fussing  about. 


PAIN   AND   INHIBITION 

Head  &  Sherren  (122)  first  described,  and  Foerster 
(77,  p.  28)  later  confirmed,  that  division  of  a  cu- 
taneous sensory  nerve  lowers  the  threshold  to  pain  in 
the  underlying  deep  tissues,  indicating  that  the  super- 
ficial system  exerts  a  moderating  influence  on  the 
threshold  and  intensity  of  deep  pain. 

Zotterman  (315)  was  one  of  the  first  to  hypothecate 
a  peripheral  inhibiting  mechanism  on  pain  within 
a  single  nerve.  He  found  only  'C  fiber  discharge  in 
the  after-stimulation  period  when  itching  occurs.  The 
relief  of  itching  by  rubbing  suggested  to  him  an 
inhibiting  action  of  fast  'A'  fibers  on  the  slower  'C 
fibers.  Landau  &  Bishop  (150)  have  extended  this 
concept  to  account  for  at  least  some  of  the  features  of 
hyperpathia  seen  in  lesions  of  peripheral  nerves.  They 
attribute  the  sensation  upon  pinprick  after  partial 
asphN'xial  compression  of  nerve  to  elimination  of 
'delta  fiber  pain.'  The  pricking  pain  then  changes,  is 


more  intense,  more  persistent  and  of  a  different  and 
burning  quality — the  result  they  say  of  release  of  the 
central  effect  of  'C  fiber  activation  normally  masked 
by  activity  of  the  delta  fibers.  We  have  already  dis- 
cussed objections  to  the  concept  that  pricking  and 
burning  pain  are  mediated  ijy  delta  and  'C  fibers, 
respectively.  However  in  those  explanations  of  hyper- 
pathia following  central  lesions  which  attribute  this 
state  to  isolated  action  of  spinoreticulothalamic, 
spinotectal  or  other  relay  routes,  there  is  also  implicit 
the  notion  that  the  divided  direct  spinothalamic 
pathway  normally  activated  by  the  same  stimulus 
exerts  an  inhibiting  action  on  the  'over-response.' 

A  number  of  obser\ations  point  to  an  inhibiting 
interaction  between  rival  stimuli  resulting  in  decrease 
of  pain.  Thus  Bender  (17)  found  that  causalgic  pain 
following  peripheral  nerve  injury  was  relieved  by  im- 
mersion of  the  opposite  normal  hand  in  water,  and 
Graham  et  al.  (108)  abolished  experimentally-induced 
itching  on  the  skin  of  the  back  by  pinprick  in  the 
same  dermatome  on  the  anterior  chest.  Hardy  et  al. 
(i  17)  demonstrated  another  form  of  inhibition  of  one 
pain  by  another  in  the  experimental  situation  of 
procaine  block  to  a  nerve  trunk.  Stimulation  of  the 
trunk  proximal  to  this  site  then  provokes  a  zone  of 
hyperalgesia.  Repeated  pinpricks  in  this  area,  how- 
ever, cause  its  borders  to  shrink;  the  investigators 
suggest  that  a  central  inhibition  is  occurring. 

Further  evidence  for  central  inhibitory  mechanisms 
for  pain  has  been  adduced  by  Foerster  (77,  pp.  77 
and  78).  In  two  patients  with  intramedullary  cord 
lesions  he  made  operative  incisions  into  portions  of 
the  posterior  columns;  a  severe  cutaneous  hyperpathia 
ensued  limited  to  those  areas  corresponding  to  the 
incised  pathways.  He  attributed  this  to  removal  of  a 
mechanism  inhibiting  pain  inherent  in  the  normal 
pathways  for  touch  and  proprioception.  In  view  of 
the  extensive  incisions  into  normal  dorsal  columns 
without  such  sequel  reported  by  Browder  &  Gallagher 
(35),  Pool  (215)  and  White  &  Sweet  (296,  p.  407), 
Foerster's  explanation  of  his  results  is  no  longer 
tenable.  His  surgery  may  have  exacerbated  effects  of 
the  original  lesion  in  gray  matter  or  nearby  postero- 
lateral white  matter.  Regarding  the  latter  possibility, 
Foerster  (77,  pp.  79  and  80)  has  cited  the  evidence  of 
Fabritius  and  Brown-Sequard  that  lesions  in  the 
deep  posterolateral  white  matter  provoke  hyperpathia, 
i.e.  that  there  is  a  corticofugal  pain  inhibiting  path- 
way in  this  region. 

An  extraordinary  patient  has  recently  been  well 
studied  by  Trent  (268).  Nearly  four  years  after  injury 
to  the  left  temporoparietal  cerebrum  the  man  began 


5O0 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPH-l'SIOLOGY    1 


to  have  pain  in  his  right  upper  limb,  maximal  in 
thumb  and  axilla.  This  on  examination  proved  to  be 
accompanied  by  "hyperalgesia  to  pin  prick,  hyper- 
esthesia to  warm  and  cold"  and  intense  pain  from  a 
vibrating  tuning  fork  when  applied  over  sharply 
defined  areas  of  the  limb  and  chest.  Both  the  spon- 
taneous pain,  and  the  abnormal  responses  to  pinprick, 
temperature  and  vibration  were  completely  and  im- 
mediately stopped  by  pressure  on  the  anterior  surfaces 
of  the  tips  of  the  medial  four  right  fingers;  later,  pres- 
sure only  on  the  tips  of  fingers  four  and  five  sufficed 
to  stop  the  pain.  The  inhibitory  mechanism  did  not 
tend  to  fatigue  and  the  patient  could  and  did  keep 
away  the  pain  by  keeping  his  fingers  clenched  to  a 
fist.  The  purely  clinical  observations  gave  no  clue  to 
the  mechanism  of  such  inhibition. 

The  converse  situation  in  which  pain  exerts  an 
inhibitory  effect  on  simultaneous  perception  of  non- 
painful  stimuli  has  been  studied  by  Benjamin  (19). 
He  found  that  several  forms  of  experimental  pain  all 
increased  the  thresholds  of  hearing  over  the  total 
tonal  range,  flicker  fusion,  vibration  at  60  cps  and 
contact  heat.  The  mean  threshold  raising  efifect  was 
generally  proportional  to  intensity  of  pain. 


REFERRED  P.iiIN 

Under  a  variety  of  circumstances  pain  arising  from 
impulses  in  one  structure,  usually  deeply  placed  such 
as  a  viscus,  is  referred  wholly  or  partly  to  some  other 
area,  usually  superficial.  The  paucity  of  nerve  endings 
in  the  deep  tissues  and  the  small  volume  of  conscious 
sensation  normally  arising  from  these  protected  areas 
allow  lesser  opportunity  for  the  cerebral  cortex  to 
build  up  a  pattern  of  the  internal  image  of  the  body 
as  detailed  as  that  of  the  surface.  And  indeed  the 
cerebral  mechanism  for  so  doing  is  much  smaller,  as 
witness  the  tiny  cerebral  cortical  area  of  splanchnic 
representation  found  by  Amassian  (5).  If  then  noxious 
stimuli  arising  from  deep  structures  converge  upon  the 
same  neuron  as  such  stimuli  from  the  skin,  the  sensory 
centers  may  refer  the  origin  of  the  stimulus  to  the  far 
more  frequent  site  of  such  origin — namely  the  skin. 

That  the  peripheral  neuron  itself  may  be  one  of  the 
sites  of  the  convergence  has  been  suggested  by  Sin- 
clair et  al.  (251).  The  bifurcation  of  a  single  parent 
axon  into  two  limbs,  each  passing  into  a  different 
nerve  trunk,  has  been  demonstrated  in  fish  by  Wern0e 
(292),  in  amphibia  by  Adrian  et  al.  (2)  and  probably 
in  mammals  by  Lloyd  (179),  but  not  yet  in  man. 
However,   the  phenomena  of  summation,  inhibition 


and  irradiation — all  demonstrable  in  connection  with 
referred  pain — are  more  readily  explicable  on  a 
central  basis.  Hence  Weddell  himself  (287)  is  inclined 
to  place  the  mechanism  for  referred  pain  mainly  in 
the  central  nervous  system. 

Ruch  (231)  has  drawn  attention  to  one  likely  site 
of  convergence  of  visceral  and  cutaneous  afferents, 
namely  the  cells  of  the  secondary  afferent  neuron  in 
the  posterior  horn,  because  he  finds  many  more  'pain 
fibers'  in  the  posterior  roots  than  axons  in  the  spino- 
thalamic tracts.  If  it  is  true  that  there  are  more 
primary  than  secondary  afferent  fibers  potentially 
concerned  with  pain,  then  two  of  the  former,  one 
from  a  viscus,  the  other  from  skin,  may  well  terminate 
in  relation  to  a  single  dorsal  horn  cell.  E.\citation  of 
the  pool  of  such  cells  from  a  viscus  ma\-  then  result  in 
erroneous  reference  to  the  skin. 

Physiologic  evidence  for  confluence  of  cutaneous 
and  deep  afferent  pathways  upon  a  single  neuron 
has  been  acquired  by  demonstration  of  firing  of  the 
central  neuron  by  either  the  cutaneous  or  the  deep 
sensory  nerve.  Proof  that  it  is  indeed  the  same  neuron 
responding  is  enhanced  by  the  finding  of  'occlusion,' 
i.e.  that  after  excitation  of  the  central  neuron  from 
one  peripheral  source  there  elapses  an  'unresponsive 
period'  during  which  it  cannot  be  excited  from  the 
other  peripheral  source.  Such  convergence  upon  single 
neural  units  in  the  thalamic  nucleus  ventralis  postero- 
lateralis  of  the  cat  has  been  found  by  MacLeod  (181) 
specifically  related  to  delta  afferent  fibers  in  the 
splanchnic  nerves  and  hence  presumptively  related 
to  pain.  In  fact  the  majority  of  the  cells  responding  to 
splanchnic  delta  afferents  also  responded  to  stimulation 
of  the  skin,  usually  that  of  the  trunk  but  at  times  that 
of  limbs  or  tail.  Such  thalamic  cells  were  present  both 
ipsilateral  and  contralateral  to  the  splanchnic 
stimulus.  The  duration  of  the  'unresponsive  period' 
of  the  pathway  depended  upon  which  peripheral 
field  was  stimulated  first,  and  the  response  to  stimu- 
lation of  one  of  the  fields  was  at  times  intermittent 
while  that  from  the  other  field  was  consistent.  Hence 
MacLeod,  cited  by  Gordon  (105),  considered  it  un- 
likely that  the  confluence  occurred  peripherally  in 
branches  of  the  primary  afferent  neuron. 

Widen  (298)  has  also  studied  in  similar  fashion 
delta  afferent  fibers  projecting  to  the  anterior  lobe  of 
the  cerebellum.  This  region  was  excited  by  stimu- 
lation of  either  a  lower  intercostal  nerve  or  the 
splanchnic  delta  fibers  and  a  high  degree  of  occlusion 
between  them  was  found.  Although  these  studies  are 
less  clearly  related  to  referred  pain  because  of  the 
lack  of  correlation  between  the  cerebellum  and  con- 


50I 


scious  sensation,  they  provide  another  example  of 
convergence  of  probable  visceral  pain  and  somatic 
afferent  fibers  upon  a  single  central  neuron. 

Clinical  observations  have  revealed  that  an  added 
state,  that  of  hyperalgesia  or  even  tenderness  upon 
pressure,  may  be  seen  in  the  area  of  skin  to  which 
pain  is  erroneously  referred.  Sinclair  el  al.  (251)  have 
explained  this,  in  their  peripheral  theory,  on  the 
basis  of  antidromic  impulses  moving  down  the  cu- 
taneous branch  of  the  parent  axon  (after  stimulation 
of  the  visceral  branch)  to  excite  secondarily  endings 
of  other  overlapping  nerve  fibers,  perhaps  via  metabo- 
lites (171,  p-  80).  Wolff  &  Hardy  (305)  after  studying 
referred  pain  and  hyperalgesia  in  experiments  on 
themselves  favor  the  theory  that  an  increasing  central 
excitatory  state  presumably  in  the  spinal  cord  is 
evoked  by  an  increasing  barrage  of  afferent  stimuli. 
They  noted,  for  example,  that  placement  of  the 
fourth  finger  in  ice  water  caused  pain  which  spread 
gradually  from  the  immersed  digit  to  those  contiguous 
to  it.  They  thought  the  referred  pain  did  not  develop 
quickly  enough  to  he  accounted  for  on  the  basis  of 
branching  primary  afferent  axons.  Moreover  pro- 
cainization  of  the  digital  nerves  in  the  painful  finger 
out  of  the  ice  water  did  not  stop  the  pain.  It  should 
have  done  so  if  antidromically  conducted  stimuli 
had  produced  an  irritating  metabolite  or  were  other- 
wise secondarily  activating  adjoining  nerves. 

However,  the  spread  of  a  central  excitatory  state 
would  account  for  the  varying  findings  following  pro- 
cainization  of  the  cutaneous  area  of  referred  pain  and 
hyperalgesia.  Procaine  injection  may  stop  these 
manifestations  (291),  perhaps  because  it  diminishes 
the  excitatory  state  at  central  cells  by  \irtue  of  stop- 
ping subthreshold  impulses  from  the  skin,  but  a 
variety  of  forms  of  experimental  pain  will  break 
through  and  cau.se  referred  pain  within  or  around 
the  anesthetic  zone  of  .skin  when  the  stimulus  becomes 
more  intense.  These  were  the  findings  of  Theobald 
(262)  with  respect  to  referred  suprapubic  pain  caused 
by  faradization  of  the  uterus,  and  of  Jones  &  Chap- 
man, cited  by  White  &  Sweet  (296,  p.  74)  relative  to 
the  cutaneous  pain  of  experimental  jejunal  disten- 
tion. In  some  studies  procainization  of  the  cutaneous 
area  of  reference  did  not  stop  the  pain  at  all;  this 
was  the  experience  of  Woollard  et  al.  (309)  whose 
direct  stimulation  of  the  phrenic  nerve  caused  shoulder 
pain,  and  of  McLellan  &  Goodell,  cited  by  White  & 
Sweet  (296,  p.  74)  who  distended  ureters  causing 
pain  in  loin  and  groin.  These  results  can  all  be  ex- 
plained by  assuming  that  the  central  excitatory  state 
from  such  visceral  stimulation  was  in  itself  adequate 


to  discharge  the  mechanism  pertinent  to  cutaneous 
reference.  Such  increase  in  the  central  excitatory  level 
may  also  arise  from  an  increase  in  impulses  from  the 
cutaneous  area.  Thus  Cohen  (47)  describes  two  pa- 
tients with  attacks  of  cardiac  anginal  pain  never 
referred  to  the  arm  in  question  until  in  one  instance 
the  inan  fractured  his  elbow  and  in  the  other  a 
blistered  area  developed  after  a  vesicant  plaster  to  the 
elbow  region.  Then  both  patients'  anginal  attacks 
included  reference  to  the  injured  elbow. 

Still  another  form  of  erroneous  reference  of  pain 
may  occur  after  injury  to  central  pathways  concerned 
therewith.  Ray  &  Wolff  (222)  have  studied  four 
patients  after  anterolateral  cordotomy  in  whom 
noxious  stimuli  of  high  intensity  in  the  analgesic  area 
induced  pain  of  low  intensity  referred  to  the  same  or 
nearby  segments  on  the  opposite  normally  innervated 
side.  The  stimuli  included  squeezing  of  muscle,  deep 
pressure  against  a  diseased  hip  joint  and  application 
of  heat  at  80°  to  90°C.  The  authors  pointed  out  that 
some  of  the  collaterals  of  entering  primary  afferent 
fibers  proceed  to  synaptic  relation  in  the  posterior 
horns  with  internuncial  neurons  whose  axons  cross 
in  the  most  dorsal  part  of  the  posterior  commissure 
to  terminate  about  cells  in  the  posterior  horns  of  the 
other  side.  The  foregoing  observations  suggest  that 
with  sufficient  background  of  facilitation  and  sufficient 
intensity  of  stimulation  such  indirect  chains  may 
transmit  impulses  which  eventually  excite  the  intact 
spinothalamic  tract  on  the  other  side  and  are  referred 
to  the  side  opposite  that  stimulated.  Inherent  in  this 
explanation  is  the  assumption  that  with  both  spino- 
thalamic pathways  functioning  before  operation,  there 
was  some  form  of  interference  with  the  internuncial 
polysynaptic  transmission.  That  such  internuncial 
relays  extend  longitudinally  in  the  cord  as  well  is 
indicated  by  our  observations  (296,  p.  257)  and  those 
of  Holbrook  &  de  Gutierrez-Mahoney  (128).  We  all 
found  that  some  patients  after  cordotomy  may  refer 
pain  to  a  much  more  rostral  segment  than  that  under- 
going intense  stimulation,  still  either  in  the  analgesic 
area  or  above  it;  the  pain  experienced  was  much 
milder  than  that  produced  in  normal  circumstances 
by  such  stimuli.  In  both  types  of  incorrect  reference 
of  the  origin  of  impulses  traversing  less  direct  path- 
ways, the  patient  interprets  them  as  though  they  had 
travelled  only  the  customary  direct  route. 

We  may  well  close  this  chapter  with  a  quotation 
from  Foerster  &  Gagel  (80).  "Pain  has  a  vital  sig- 
nificance; it  is  no  wonder  then  that  those  physical 
processes  associated  with  the  psychic  experience  of 
pain   have  the  broadest  anatomical   basis.   The  im- 


502 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


pulses  leading  to  pain  penetrate  to  and  upwards  within 
the  central  nervous  system  by  a  thousand  devious 
routes.  The  wisdom  of  nature,  which  has  placed  pain 
as  the  guardian  over  life  and  health,  has  provided 


it  with  many  paths  and  many  back  doors."  Even  the 
main  avenues  and  mechanisms  for  pain  are  still 
poorly  understood;  the  task  of  the  neurophysiologist 
in  this  field  lies  largely  before  him. 


R  ri  F  E  R  E  N  C  E  S 


3 
4 
5 
6 

r 

8, 

9 
lo. 

1 1. 
i-i. 

13- 

14. 

'5- 
16. 


18. 

'9- 
■20. 
21. 

22. 

■^3- 

24- 
25- 

26. 

27- 

28. 


30. 
3'- 
32- 

33- 

34- 


Adrian,  E.  D.  1  he  Basis  of  Seusalion.  London:  Christophers, 

1928. 

Adrian,  E.  D.,  McK.  Cattell  and  H.  Ho.^gland,  J. 

Physiol.  72:377,  1 93 1. 

Adson,  a.  W.  Proc.  Staff  Meet.  Mayo  Clin.    10:  623,    1935. 

Alrutz,  S.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.  17:414,  1905. 

Amassian,  V.  E.  J.  .Neurophysiot .  14:  445,   1951. 

Amassian,  V.  E.  .4.  Res.  .Ven:  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  371, 

1952- 

Arieff,  a.  J.,  S.  VV.   PvziK  and  E.  L.  Tigav.  .-l.M.A. 

Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  77:  156,  1957. 

Aristotle.   Nicomachean   Ethics,   translated   by   Sir   David 

Ross.  London:  Oxford,  1954.  Bk.  II,  3. 

.ARM.STRONO,  D.,  R.  M.  L.  Dry,  C  A.  Keele  and  J.  W. 

Markham.  j'.  Physiol.  120:326,  1953. 

.Armstrong,  D.,  J.  B.  Jepson,  C.  A.  Keele  and  J.  VV. 

Stewart.  J.  Physiol.  135:  350,  1957. 

AsHBV,  M.  Brain  72:  599,  1949. 

Bailed',  P.,  J.  G.  Dusser  de  Barenne,  H.  \V.  Garol  and 

W.  S.  McCuLLOCH.  J.  .\curophysiol.  3:  469,  1940. 

Balchum,    O.   J.    AND    H.    M.    Weaver.    A.M. A.    Arch. 

jVeurol.  &  Psychiat.  49:  739,  1943. 

Banzet,  p.  M.  La  Cordotomie.   Etude  Anatomique,   Technique. 

Clinique,  et  Physiologique.  Paris:  Arnette,  1927. 

Beecher,  H.  V^.  J. A.m. a.  161:  1609,  1956. 

Beecher,  H.  K.  Pharmacol.  Rev.  9:  59,  1957. 

Bender,    M.    B.    .i.M.A.   Arch.   .Neurol.  &   Psychiat.   54:    i, 

■945- 

Benj.amin,  F.  B.  J.  Appl.  Physiol.  5:  740,  1953. 

Benjamin,  F.  B.  J.  Appl.  Physiol.  8 :  630,  1 956. 

Benjamin,  F.  B.  J.  Invest.  Dermat.  26:  471,  1956. 

Bentlev,  F.  H.  Ann.  Surg.  128:881,  1948. 

Bentlev,  F.  H.  and  R.  H.  Smitiiwick.  Lancet  239:  389, 

1940. 

Bethe,  a.  Arch,  mihoskop.  .inat.  44:  185,  1894. 

Bickford,  R.  G.  Clin.  Sc.  3:  337,  1938. 

Biemond,   .a.   A..M..i.   Arch.   .Xcurol.  &   P.ychiat.    75:   231, 

1956. 

BiGELOw,   N.,    I.   Harrison,   H.    Goodell   and    H.    G. 

Wolff.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  24:  503,  1945. 

Bishop,  G.  H.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  361,  1943. 

Bishop,  G.  H.  and  P.  Heinbecker.  Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  & 

Med.  26:  241,  1928. 

BoK,   S.  T.   In:    Handhuch  der  Mikroskopischen   .Inalomie  des 

Menschen,  edited  by  W.  von  Mollendorff.  Berlin:  Springer, 

1929,  vol.  IV. 

Boring,  E.  G.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  10:  1,  191 6. 

Bowsher,  D.  Brain  80:  606,  1957. 

BovD,  D.  a.,  Jr.  and  L.  W.  Nie.  A.M.A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 

Psychiat.  61:  402,  1949. 

Br  ASHE  ar,  a.  D.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  64:  169,  1936. 

Brookhart,  J.  M.,  W.  K.  Living.ston  and  F.  P.  Haugen. 

J.  .Neurophysiol.  16:  634,  1953. 


35.  Browder,  E.J.  andJ.  p.  Gallagher.  Ann.  .Surg.  128:  456, 
1948. 

36.  Bullock,  T.  H.  Fed.  Proc.  l2:  666,  1953. 

37.  Cadvvalder,  W.  B.  and  J.  E.  Sweet.  J..A..M.A.  58:  1490, 
1912. 

38.  Cannon,  B.  .im.  J.  Physiol.  105:  366,  1933. 

39.  Chapman,  VV.  P.  Psychosom.  .Med.  6:  252,  1944. 

40.  Chapman,  VV.  P.  and  C.  N.  Jones.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  23:  81, 
1944. 

41.  Chapm.\n,  VV.  P.,  .\.  S.  Rose  and  H.  C.  Solomon.  A.  Res. 
.Nerv.  &  Ment.  Vis.,  Proc.  2y.  754,  1948. 

42.  Clark,  D,  J.  Hughes  and  H.  S.  Gasser.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 
"4:69,  1935. 

43.  Clark,  W.  E.  J.  Anat.  71:7,  1936. 

44.  Clausen,  J.  and  H.  E.  King.  J.  Phychol.  30:  299,   1950. 

45.  Cleveland,  D.  A.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  54:  35,  1932. 

46.  Cobb,  S.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  2:  505,  1919. 

47.  Cohen,  H.  7>.  M.  Soc.  London  64:  35,  1944. 

48.  Collins,  W.  F,  and  J.  L.  O'Learv.  ElectroerKcphalog.  & 
Clin.  .Neurophysiol.  6:  619,  1954. 

49.  Collins,   W.   F.   and   C.   T.   Randt.  J.  .Neurophysiol.    19: 
438,  1956. 

50.  Crawford,  A.  S.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Surg.  55:  523,  1947. 

51.  Critchlev,  M.  Brit.  M.  J.  2:  891,  1934. 

52.  Dandy,  W.  E.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  53:  31,  1933. 

53.  Darwin,  E.  ^odnomia,  or  The  Laws  of  Organic  Lije.  London : 
J.Johnson,  1794,  vol.   i. 

54.  Davis,  L.,  J.  T.  Hart  and  R.  C.  Grain.  Surg.  Gynec.  & 
Obst.  48:  647,  1929. 

55.  Davis,   L.   and   L.  J.   Pollock.   A..\i.A.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  27:282,  1932. 

56.  Davis,  L.,  L.  J.  Pollock  .and  T.  T.  Sto.ne.  .Surg.  Gynec.  & 
Obst.  55:418,  1932- 

57.  Davison,    C.    and   W.    Schick.    A..\I..i.   Arch.   .Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  34:  1204,  1935. 

58.  Dawson,  G.  D.  and  J.  VV.  Scott.  J.  Neurol.  Newosurg.  G? 
Psychiat.  12:  259,  1949. 

59.  Dearborn,   G.   van   N.  J    .Nerv.  &  Ment.   Dis.   75:  612, 

1932- 

60.  Dejerine,    J.    .and   J.    Mouzon.    Rev.    neurol.    28:    1265, 

19"5- 

61.  Dejerine,  J.  and  G.  Roussv.  Rev.  neurol.  14:  521,  1906. 

62.  DELG.ADO,  J.  M.  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18:  261,  1955. 

63.  Denton,  J.  E.  and  H.  K.  Beecher.  J. A.M. A.  141  :  1031, 

1949- 

64.  D'Errico,  A.  J.  Neurosurg.  7:  294,  1950. 

65.  de    Spinoza,    B.    The   Philosophy   oj  Spinoza.    New    York: 
Carlton  House,  1927,  p.  217. 

66.  DooiEL,  .A.  S.  Arch,  mikroskop.  .-inat.  52:  44,  1898. 

67.  Dogliotti,  M.  Anesth.  et  analg.   17:  143,  1938. 

68.  DouPE,  J.,  C.  H.  CuLLEN  and  G.  Q.  Chance.  J.  .Neurol. 
.Neurosurg.  &  Psychiat.  7 :  33,  1 944. 


PAIN 


503 


69.  Drake,   C.   G.   and  K.  G.  McKenzie.  J.  jVeurosurg.   10: 

457.  1953- 

70.  DussER  DE  Barenne,  J.  G.  In:  Handbuch  der  .Veurologie, 
edited  by  Bumke  and  Foerster.  Berlin:  Springer,  1937, 
vol.  2,  p.  268. 

71.  EcHLiN,  F.  J.  .\'eurosurg.  6:  530,  1949. 

72.  EcHLiN,  F.  AND  A.  Fessard.  J.  Physiol.  93:  312,  1938. 

73.  EiCHLER,   W.   ^Ischr.   Psychol.   Physiol.   Sinnesorg.   60:    325, 

1930- 

74.  Erickso.n,  T.  C,  W.  J.  Bleckwenn  and  C.  N.  Woolsey. 
7>.  Am.  Neurol.  A.  77:  57,  1952. 

75.  Falconer,   M.   A.  J.   .\eurot.  .Heurosurg.  &   Psychial.    1 2 : 

297.  '949- 

76.  Feldberc,  W.  and  S.  L.  Sherwood.  J.  Physiol.  125:  488, 

'954- 

77.  Foerster,  O.  Die  Leitungsbahnen  des  Schmerzgefuhls  und  die 
chiruTgische  Behandlung  der  Schmercz^stdnde.  Berlin :  Urban, 
1927. 

78.  Foerster,  O.  In :  Handbuch  der  .Veurologie,  edited  by 
Bumke  and  Foerster.   Berlin:  Springer,    1936,   vol.    6,   p. 

358. 

79.  Foerster,  O.,  H.  Altenburger  and  F.  \V.  Kroll. 
^tschr.ges.  .Neurol.  Psychial.  121  :  139,  1929. 

80.  Foerster,  O.  .and  O.  Gagel.  ^Ischr.  ges.  .Neurol.  Psychial. 
138:  I,  1932. 

81.  Ford,  F.  R.  and  L.  Wilkins.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  62: 
448,  1938. 

82.  Frazier,  C.  H.  A.m. a.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychial.  19:  650, 
1928. 

83.  Frazier,  C.  H.  and  W.  G.  Spiller.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 
Psychial.  9:1,  1923. 

84.  Freeman,  W.  and  J.  Watts.  Psychosurgery  in  Ihe  Trealment 
of  Menial  Disorders  &  Inlraclable  Pain  (2nd  ed.^.  Spring- 
field ;  Thomas,  1 950. 

85.  French,  J.  D.,  E.  K.  von  Amerongen  and  H.  \V. 
Magoun.  A.M. a.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychial.  68:  577,  1952. 

86.  Gad,  J.  and  A.  Goldscheider.  ^Ischr.  klin.  .Med.  20:  339, 
1892. 

87.  Gardner,  E.  and  H.  M.  Cuneo.  .4..V/..-1.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 
Psychial.  53:423.  '945- 

88.  Gardner,  W.  J.,  L.  J.  Karnosch,  C.  C.  McClure,  Jr. 
and  Ann  K.  Gardner.  Brain  78:  487,  1955. 

89.  Gasser,  H.  S.  a.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:  44, 

'943- 

90.  Gasser,  H.  S.  Les  Prix  .Nobel  en  i()40-i()44.  Stockholm: 
Norstedt,  1946,  p.  128. 

91.  Gasser,  H.  S.  and  J.  Erlanger.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  88:  581, 

1929- 

92.  Gasser,  H.  S.  .and  H.   Grundfest.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.    127: 

393.  '939- 

93.  Gaze,  R.   M.   and  G.   Gordon.   Quart.  J.  Exprr.  Physiol. 

39:  279.  ■954- 

94.  Gaze,  R.  M.  .and  G.  Gordon.  Quart.  J.  Exper.  Physiol.  40: 

'87.  1955- 

95.  Gerard,  M.  \V.  ,-1..\/..1.  .^rrA.  .\curol.  &  Psychial.  9:  306, 

1923- 

96.  Gernandt,  B.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta,  physiol.  scandinav. 
12:  56,  1946. 

97.  Gless,  p.  J.  .Neuropath.  &  Exper.  Neurol.  5 :  54,  1 946. 

98.  Glees,  P.  Acta  neuroveg.  7:  160,  1953. 

99.  Glees,  P.  and  R.  A.  Baile\'.  Monals.schr.  Psychial.  u.  Neurol. 
122:  129,  1951. 


100.   Goetzl,  F.  R.,  C.  W.  Bien  and  G.  Lu.  J.  Appl.  Physiol. 

4:  161,  1951. 
loi.   Goldscheider,  A.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  Suppl.  i:  1,  1885. 

102.  Goldscheider,  A.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  39:  96,  1886. 

103.  Goldstein,  K.  .Neurol,  ^enlralbl.  29:  898,  1910. 

104.  Gooddy,  VV.  Brain  80:  1 18,  1957. 

105.  Gordon,  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  50:  586,  1957. 

106.  Gordon,    G.    and    D.    Whitteridge.    Lancet    245:    700, 

1943- 

107.  Gorham,  L.  \V.  a.  Res.  Nen:  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:  337, 
1942. 

108.  Graham,  D.  T.,  H.  Goodell  and  H.  G.  Wolff.  J.  Clin. 
Invest.  30:  37,  1 95 1. 

109.  Granit,  R.,  L.  Leksell  and  C.  R.  Skoglund.  Brain  67: 

125,  1944- 

1 10.  Grant,  F.  C.  Ann.  Surg.  92:  998,  1930. 

111.  Grantham,  E.  G.  J.  Neurosurg.  8:  405,   1951. 

112.  Grimson,   K.   S.,   F.   H.   Hesser   and   W.   W.   Kitchin. 
Surgery  22:  230,    1947. 

113.  Habgood,  J.  S.J.  Physiol,   in:  195,   1950. 

114.  Haggqvist,  G.  Z^schr.  mikroskop.-anat.  Forsch.  39:  I,   1936. 

115.  Hardy',  J.  D.,  H.  G.  Wolff  and  H.  Goodell.  J.  Clin. 
Invest.    19:    649,    1940. 

116.  Hardy,  J.  D.,  H.  G.  Wolff  and  H.  Goodell.  A.  Res. 
Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:    i,    1943. 

117.  Hardy,  J.  D.,  H.  G.  Wolff  and  H.  Goodell.  J.  Clin. 
Invest.  29:  115,   1950. 

118.  Hardy,  J.  D.,   H.   G.   Wolff   and  H.   Goodell.   Pain 
Sensations  and  Reactions.   Baltimore:  Williams   &   Wilkins, 

1952- 

1 19.  Harris,  W.  Brit.  M.  J.  2:  112,  1936. 

120.  Harrison,  F.  and  K.  B.  Corbin.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  5:  465, 

1942- 

121.  Head,  H.,  W.  H.  R    Rivers  and  J.  .Sherren.  Brain  28: 

99.  igoo- 

122.  Head,  H.  and  J.  Sherren.  Brain  28:  116,  1905. 

123.  Hecaen,   H.   and  J.   DE   .AjuRiAGUERRA.   Rev.   neurol.   83: 
300,  1950. 

124.  Hecaen,  H.,  J.  Tal.airach,  M.  David  and  M.  B.  Dell. 
Rev.  neurol.  81;  917,    1949. 

125.  Heinbecker,  p.,  G.  H.  Bishop  and  J.  OLe.ary.  A.M. A. 
Arch.   .Neurol.  &   Psychial.   29:    771,    1933. 

126.  Helson,  H.   Brain  55:   114,    1932. 

127.  Herzen,  a.  Arch.  ges..  Physiol.  38:  93,   1886. 

128.  HOLBROOK,    T.    J.     AND     C.     G.     DE     GuTIERREZ-MaHONEY. 

Fed.  Proc.   6:    131,    1947. 

129.  Horrax,   G.   Surgery  20:  593,    1946. 

130.  HoRSLEY,  V.  Brit.  M.  J.  2:  125,  1909. 

131.  Hurst  (formerly  Hertz),   A.   F.   Lancet  89:    1051,    11 19, 
1 187,  191 1. 

132.  Hyndman,  O.  R.  J.  Internal.  Coll.  Surgeons  5:  394,  1942. 

133.  Hyndman,  O.   R.   and   J.  Wolkin.   A..M..i.  Arch.  Neurol. 
&  Psychial.   50:   129,    1943. 

134.  James,  W.    The  Principles  oj  Psychology.    New  York:  Holt, 
1890. 

135.  Javert,  C.  T.  and  J.  D.  Hardy.  Anesthesiology  12:   189, 

I95I- 

136.  Jeutsbury,  E.  C.  O.  Brain  74:  336,   1951. 

137.  Jones,  M.  H.  Science  124:  442,  1956. 

138.  Karplus,   I.   P.   AND   .\.  Kreidl.   Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  207: 
134.  ■92.'5- 


504 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


139.  Katsuki,  Y.,  S.   Yoshino  and  J.   Chen.  Jap.  J.  Physiol. 

1  :  179'  I95>- 

140.  Kf.llgren,  J.  H.  Clin.  Sc.  4:  35,  1939- 

141.  King,  R.  B.  .Neuroln^y  7:  610,   1957- 

142.  KiNSELLA,  V.    ].   Sril.  J.  Surg.   27:  449,    1940. 

143.  Kleist,    K.     In:    Handhuch   der    ArzH'chen   Erjahrungen    tm 
It'ellkriege.  Leipzig:  Barth,   1934,  vol.  4,  pt.  2,  p.   1416. 

144.  KoENiGSTEiN,  H.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Dermal.  &  Syph.  57:  828, 
1948. 

145.  KosKOFF,   Y.   D.,   W.   Dennis,   D.    Lazovik   and   E.   T. 
Wheeler.  A.  Res.  JVerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  27:  901,  1948. 

1 46.  Kra'S'enbuhl,   H.    and    VV.    A.    Stoll.   Ada  neiirochir.    i : 

1.  1950- 

147.  Kroll,  F.  \V.  ^tschr.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.  128:  751,    1930. 

148.  KuNKLE,   E.    C.    and  W.   p.   Chapman.   A.   Res.  Nerv.   & 
Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:  100,   1943. 

149.  KuRU,  M.  Sensory  Paths  in  the  Spinal  Cord  and  Brain  Stem 
of  Man.  Tokyo:  So^ensya,  1949. 

150.  Landau,  W.  M.  and  G.  H.  Bishop.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  69:  490,  1953. 

151.  Landis,   E.   M.   Heart   15:  209,    1930. 

152.  Langlev,  J.    N.    Phil.    Trans.   Roy.  Soc,   London    183:    114, 
1892. 

153.  Langlev,  J.  N.  J.  Physiol.  20:  55,  1896. 

154.  Langlev,  J.  N.  J.  Phvsiol.  25:  468,  1900. 

155.  Langlev,  J.  N.  Brain  26:   i,   1903. 

156.  Langlev,  J.  N.  Lancet  207:  955,   1924. 

157.  Lanier,  L.  H.,  H.  M.  Carnev  and  W.  D.  Wilson.  A.M. A. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.   34:    1,    1935. 

158.  Lavrenko,   V.   V.   Bull.   E.xper.   Biol.    .\led.,    U.S.S.R.   (in 
Russian).  5:  37,  1938. 

159.  LeBeau,   J.    Psycko-chirurgie    et    Fonctions    .Mcntales.    Paris: 
Masson,    1954,   p.   429. 

160.  Leduc,   E.   H.   and  D.   Slaughter.  Anesth.  &  Analg.  24; 

147.  ■945- 

161.  Lele,  p.  p.  and  D.  C;.  Sinclair.  J.  .Neurol.  .Neurosurg.  & 

Psychiat.   18:   120,   1955. 

162.  Lele,  P.  P.,  D.  C.  Sinclair  and  G.  Weddell.  J.  Physiol. 
123:  187,  1954. 

163.  Lele,  P.  P.  and  G.  Weddell.  Brain  79:  119,  1956. 

164.  Lennander,  K.  G.  ^entralbl.  Chir.  28:  209,   1901. 

165.  Lennander,    K.    G.    Mitt.    Grenzg.    Med.    Chir.    15:    465, 
1906. 

166.  Leriche,  R.  Presse  rued.  45:  971,   1937. 

167.  Leriche,   R.    La   Chirurgie  de  la   Doiileur.    Paris:   Masson 

1949- 

168.  Leriche,  R.  and  R.  Fontaine.  Ren.  neurol.  32:  483,  1925. 

169.  Lewin,  W.   and  C.  G.   Phillips.  J.  Neurol.  .Neurosurg.  & 
Psychiat.    15:   143,    1952. 

1 70.  Lewis,  T.  Brit.  M.  J.   1:321,  1 938. 

171.  Lewis,  T.  Pain.  New  York:  Macmillan,   1942,  p.    187. 

172.  Lewis,  T.  and  W.  Hess.  Clin.  Sc.  i  :  39,  1933. 

173.  Lewis,  T.,  G.  \V.   Pickering  and  P.  Rothschild.  Heart 
16:   I,    1931. 

174.  Lewis,  T.  and  E.  E.  Pochin.  Clin.  Sc.  3:  67,  1937. 

175.  Lewis,  T.  and  E.  E.  Pochin.  Clin.  Sc.  3:  141,   1938. 

176.  LiNDGREN,   L   and  H.   Olivecrona.  J.  .Neurosurg.  4:   19, 

1947- 

177.  Livingston,  W.  K.  J.  Neurosurg.  4:  140,  1947. 

178.  Lloyd,  D.  P.  C.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  293,  1943. 

179.  Llovd,  D.   p.   C.  Physiol.  Rev.  24:   i,   1944. 

180.  LuFT,  R.  and  H.  Olivecrona.  Cancer  8:  261,   1955. 


181.  MacLeod,  J.  G.J.  Physiol.  133:  16P,  1956. 

182.  Madonick,  M.J.  Neurology  4:  554,  1954. 

183.  Magladery  J,.  W.,  D.  B.  McDougal,  Jr.  and  J.  Stole. 
Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  86:  291,    1950. 

184.  Marshall,  J.  J.  Neurol.  .Neurosurg.  &  Psychiat.    14:   187, 

195'- 

185.  Maruhashi,  J.,  K.  Mizuguchi  and  L  Tasaki.  J.  Physiol. 

■17:  129.   1952- 

186.  McKinlev,  W.  .\.  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

'37:   2'7.    '942- 

187.  Mehler,  W.  R.  Anat.  Rec.   127:  332,   1957. 

188.  Mehler,  W.  R.,  M.  E.  Feferman  and  VV.  J.  H.  Naut.'V. 
Anat.  Rec.  124:332,  1956. 

189.  Michelsen,  J.  J.  .4.  Re^.  .Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  23: 
86,  1943. 

190.  Morin,  F.,  H.  G.  Schwartz  and  J.  L.  O' Lear  v.  Acta 
psychiat.  et  neurol.  scandinav.   26 :  371,    1 95 1 . 

191.  Morison,  R.  S.  and  E.  \V.  Dempsev.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 
135:  281,  1942. 

192.  Moritz,  a.   R.  .^nd  F.  C.  Henriques,   Jr.  Am.  J.  Path. 

^r-  695-  1947- 

193.  Morton,  D.  R.,  K.  P.  Kl.-vssen  and  G.  M.  Curtis. 
Surgery  30 :  800,   1 95  I . 

194.  Moruzzi,  G.  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  Electroencephalog.  & 
Clin.  .Neurophysiol.    i:  455,    1949. 

195.  Mott,   F.   W.   Brain    15:  215,    1892. 

196.  Mountcastle,  V.  B.,  M.  R.  Covian  and  C.  R.  Harrison. 
.4.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  339,   1952. 

197.  Mueller,  E.  E.,  R.  Loeffel  and  S.  Mead.  J.  Appl. 
Physiol.    5:    746,    1953. 

198.  Nafe,  J.  P.  In;  Handbook  of  General  Experimental  Psychology, 
edited  by  C.  A.  Murchison.  Worcester:  Clark  Univ. 
Press,    1934,  chapt.   20. 

199.  Nauta,  W.  J.   H.   AND   P.   A.   Gvgax.  Slain    Technol.   29: 

91.  '954- 

200.  NicOLESCO,  J.  Compt.  rend.  .Soc.  de  biol.   115:  1556,   1934. 

201.  O'Brien,   J.    L.    and    E.    S.    Goldensohn.    Neurology    7: 

549.  1957- 

202.  Olszewski,  J.  J.  Comp.  .Veurol.  92:  401,    1950. 

203.  Oppel,  T.  W.  .■^nd  J.  D    Hard-i'.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  16:  525, 

'937- 

204.  Orchinik,  C,  R.  Koch,  H.  T.  Wvcis,  H.  Freed  and 
E.  S.  Speigel.  a.  Res.  Nerv.  &  .Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  29:  172, 
1950. 

205.  Partridge,  M.  Pre-Fronlal  Leucotomy.  Springfield :  Thomas, 

■950. 

206.  Pattle,  R.  E.  and  G.  Weddell.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  11:  93, 
1948. 

207.  Pearson,    A.    A.    A..M..i.    .Arch.    .Neurol.   &    Psychiat.    68: 

515.  '952- 

208.  Peet,  M.  M.  A.m. a.  Arch.  Surg.   13:   153,  1926. 

209.  Peet,  M.   M.   A..M..4.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  22:  313, 

'929- 

210.  Penfield,  W.  and  E.  Boldrey.  Brain  60:  389,  1937. 

211.  Penfield,  W.  and  T.  Rasmussen.  The  Cerebral  Cortex 
of  Man.  New  York;  Macmillan,   1950,  p.  44. 

212.  Pieron,  H.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  103:  883,  1930. 

213.  Poirier,  L.  J.  and  C.  Bertrand.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.    102: 

745.  '955- 

214.  Pollock,  L.  J.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  2:  667, 

1919. 

215.  Pool,  J.  L.  .-Inn.  Surg.  124:  386,  1946. 


PAIN 


505 


216.  Pool,   J.   L.    TV.  &  Slud.  Colt.   Physicians  Philadelphia    19: 

49.  195'- 

217.  Pritchard,  E.  a.  B.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  26:  697,  1933. 

218.  Ranson,   S.  W.  J.A.M.A.  86:   1887,    1926. 

219.  Ranson,  S.  W.   and   P.   R.   Bii.lincslev.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 
40:  571,   1916. 

220.  Rasmussen,   a.  T.   and  W.   T.   Peyton.  Surgery   10:  699, 
1941. 

221.  Ray,  B.  S.  and  C.  L.  Neill.  Ann.  Surg.  126:  709,  1947. 
221.   Ray,  B.  S.   and  H.   G.  Wolff.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 

Psycluat.  53:  257,    1945. 

223.  Rivers,  W.  H.  R.  and  H.  Head.  Brain  31:  323,  1908. 

224.  Roe,  W.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  43:  250,   1950. 

125.   RosENBACH,  O.  Deutsche  med.   Wchnschr.   10:  338,   1884. 

226.  Rosenthal,    S.    R.    Proc.    Soc.    Exper.    Biol.  &    Med.    74: 
167,   1950. 

227.  RoTHMAN,  S.  .\rch.  Dermal,  u.  Syph.  139:  227,  1922. 

228.  RoTHMAN,  S.  A.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Merit.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:   iio, 

1943- 

229.  RoussY,  G.   Rev.  neiirot.    17:  301,   1909. 

230.  Rubins,  J.  L.  and  E.  D.  Friedman.  A..\I.A.  Arch.  .Neurol. 
&  Psychiat.  60:  554,  1948. 

231.  RucH,  T.   C.  .-1    Textbook  of  Physiologx  (i6th  ed.),  edited 
by  J.  F.  Fulton.  Philadelphia:  Saunders,   1949,  p.  360. 

232.  RucH,  T.  C,  H.  D.  Patton  and  V.  E.  Amassian.  A.  Res. 
Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  30:  403,   1952. 

233.  Russell,  W.  R.  Brain  68:  79,  1945. 

234.  Ry'Lander,  G.  a.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  27:  691, 
1948. 

235.  ScARFF,  J.  E.  J.  Neurosurg.  7:  330,  1950. 

236.  Schacter,  M.  Praxis  45:  681,   1956. 

237.  ScHAMP,  J.  R.  and  H.  M.  Schamp.  J.  Dent.  Res.  25:  loi, 
1946. 

238.  ScHiFF,  J.    M.    In:   Lehrhuch   der   Physiologic  des   Menschen. 
Lahr :  Schauenbuig,    1 858,   vol.    i . 

239.  ScHILDER,  P.  and  E.  Stengel.  ^tschr.  ges.  .Neurol.  Psychiat. 

113:    '43,    1928. 

240.  Schilder,   p.   and   E.   Stengel.   A.M.A.  Arch.  Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.   25:  598,    1931. 

241.  Schumacher,   G.    A.,   H.    Goodell,  J.    D.    Hardy   and 
H.    G.    Wolff.   Science   92:    110,    1940. 

242.  Schwartz,   H.   G.   and  J.   L.   O'Leary.   Surgery  9:    183, 
1941. 

243.  Schwartz,    H.    G.    and    J.    L.    O'Leary.    A.M.A.    Arch. 
Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  47:  293,   1942. 

244.  Scoville,  W.   B.,   E.   K.  Wilk  and  A.  J.   Pepe.  Am.  J. 
Psychiat.  1 07 :  730,   1 95 1  • 

245.  Sharpey-Schafer,    E.    (^imrt.  J.   Exper.   Physiol.    19:   85, 
1928. 

246.  Sicard,  J.  A.  and  Robineau.  Rev.  neural.  1:21,  1925. 

247.  Sinclair,  D.  C.  Brain  78:  584,  1955. 

248.  Sinclair,  D.  C.  and  J.  R.  Hinshaw.  Brain  73:  224,  1950. 

249.  Sinclair,  D.  C.  and  J.  R.  Hinshaw.  Brain  73:  480,  1950. 

250.  Sinclair,  D.   C.   and  J.   R.  Hinshaw.   Quart.  J.  Exper. 
Psychol.    3:   49,    1951. 

251.  Sinclair,    D.    C.,    G.    Weddell   and    W.    H.    Feindel. 
Brain   71:    1 84,    1 948. 

252.  Slaughter,  D.  and  F.  T.  Wright.  Anesth.  &  Analg.  23: 
115,   1944. 

253.  Spiegel,  E.  A.  and  W.   |.  Bernis.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  210: 
209,  1925. 


254.  Starzl,   T.    E.    and   D.    G.    Whitlock.   J.   Neurophystol. 

15:   449.    '952- 

255.  Stookey,  B.  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  69:  552,   1929. 

256.  Strughold,  H.  ^tschr.  Biol.  80:  367,   1924. 

257.  Sutton,  D.  C.  and  H.  C.  Lueth.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Int.  Med. 
45:  827,   1930. 

258.  Swank,   R.   L.   and  H,   A.   Davenport.  Slain   Technol.  g: 

129.  1934- 

259.  Sweet,  W.  H.  J. .A.M. A.  142:  392,  1950. 

260.  Talairach,  J.,   H.   Hecaen,   M.   David,   M.   Monnier 
AND  J.  DE  Ajuriaouerra.  Rcv.  ncurol.  81:  4,   1949. 

261.  Taylor,   C.   W.  J.  Neurosurg.    11:  508,    1954. 

262.  Theobald,  G.  W.  Lancet  257:  41,    1949. 

263.  Thole.  .Neurol,  ^entratbl.   31:  610,    1912. 

264.  Thunberg,  T.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiol.    12:  394,    1902. 

265.  Tindall,  G.  T.  and  E.  C.  Kunkle.  A.M.A.  Arch.  .Neurol. 
&  Psychiat.   77:  605,    1957. 

266.  ToROK,    L.   ^tschr.    Psychol.   Physiol.  Sinnesorg.   Abt.    II    46: 
23,  1907. 

267.  Tower,  Sarah  S.  A.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  23: 
16,  1943. 

268.  Trent,  S.  E.  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  123:  356,  1956. 

269.  Trotter,  W.  Brit.  M.  J.  2:  103,  1926. 

270.  Trotter,  W.  and  H.  M.  Da  vies.  J.  Physiol.  38:  134,  1909. 

271.  Van  Gehuchten,  A.  Nevraxe  3:  237,  1901. 

272.  VAN   Harreveld,   a.  and  H.   M.   Smith.  J.  Neurophystol. 

'5-    313.    1952- 

273.  VON  Frey,  M.  Ber.  sacks.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  46:  185,  1894. 

274.  von  Fre\-,  M.  Ber.  sacks.  Gesellsch.  Wiss.  48:  175,  1896. 

275.  Walker,   A.    E.    The  Primate    Thalamus.    Chicago:   Univ. 
Chicago  Press,   1938. 

276.  Walker,  A.  E.  .A..\t.A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  43:  284, 
1940. 

277.  Walker,  A.  E.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Surg.  44:  953,   1942. 

278.  Walker,    A.    E.    A.M.A.    Arch.    Neurol.   &    Psychiat.    48: 
884,  1942. 

279.  Walker,  A.  E.  A.  Res.  .Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.,  Proc.  23:  63, 

■943- 

280.  Walker,  A.  E.  and  F.  Nulsen.  ^•l..l/..l.  Arch.  .Neurol.  & 
Psychiat.  59:559,  1948. 

281.  Wallenberg,  A.  Arch.  Psyckiat.  34:  923,    190!. 

282.  Walshe,   F.   M.   R.   Brain  65:  48,    1942. 

283.  Waterston,   D.  J.  Pkysiol.   77:  251,    1933. 

284.  Waterston,  D.  Lancet  224:  943,  1933. 

285.  Weddell,  G.  J.  Anat.  75:  346,   1941. 

286.  Weddell,   G.  J.  Anat.  75:  441,    1941. 

287.  Weddell,  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  50:  581,   1957. 

288.  Weddell,    G.,   W.    Pallie   and    E.    Palmer.    Qiiart.   J. 
Microsc.  Sc.  95:  483,    1954. 

289.  Weddell,  G.,  D.  C.  Sinclair  and  W.  H.  Feindel.  J. 
Neurophystol.    1 1  :   99,    1 948. 

290.  Weinstein,  E.  a.,  R.  L.  Kahn  and  W.  H.  Slote.  A.M.A. 
Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.   74:  235,    1955. 

291.  Weiss,  S.  and  D.  Davis.  Am.  J.  M.  Sc.  176:  517,  1928. 

292.  Wern0e,  T.  B.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  210:   i,   1925. 

293.  White,  J.  C.  A.M.A.  Arch.  Surg.  43:  113,  1941. 

294.  White,  J.  C.  .\nd  E.  F.  Bland.  Medicine  27:  i,  1948. 

295.  White,  J.  C,  W.  E.  Garrey  and  J.  A.  Atkins.  A.M.A. 
Arch.   Surg.    26:    765,    1933. 

296.  White,  J.  C.  and  W.  H.  Sweet.  Pain:  Its  Mechanisms  and 
Neurosurgical  Control.  Springfield:  Thomas,    1955. 

297.  Whyte,  H.  M.  Clin.  Sc.  10:  333,  1951. 


5o6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


■298.   Widen,  L.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  33:  Suppl.   117,   1955- 

299.  Wilson,  G.  and  T.  Va\.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Neurol,  &  Psychiat. 
22 :  638,  1929. 

300.  Wilson,  J.  G.  Am.  J.  Anal.   11:   loi,   191  i. 

301.  Wilson,  S.  A.  K.  Neurology  (2nd  ed.),  edited  by  A.  N. 
Bruce.  London:  Butterworth,    1955,  vol.   II,  p.   703. 

302.  Winkler,  C.  Manuel  de  Neurologte.  Haarlem:  Bohn,   1921, 
vol.   I,  pt.  2,  p.  51. 

303.  Wolff,  H.  G.  Headache  and  Other  Pain.  New  York:  Oxford, 
1948. 

304.  Wolff,  H.  G.  and  H.  Goodell.  .-1.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment. 
Dis.,  Proc.  23:  434,  1943. 

305.  Wolff,  H.   G.  and  J.  D.   Hardv.   Physiol.  Rer.  27:  167, 

■947- 


306.  Wolff,  H.  G.,  J.   D.   Hardv  and  H.  Goodell.  J.  Clin. 
Invest.    19:   659,    1940. 

307.  WooLLARD,  H.  H.   Brain  58:  352,    1935. 

308.  WoOLLARD,  H.  H.  J.  Anat.  Ji:  480,   1937. 

309.  WooLLARD,    H.    H.,    J.    E.    H.    Roberts    and    E.    A. 
Carmiciiael.  Lancet  222:  337,   1932. 

310.  Woollard,   H.   H.,   G.   Weddell  and  J.   .\.   Harpman. 
J.  Anat.  74:413,  1940. 

311.  WooLSEV,  C.  N.  and  D.  Fairman.  Surgery  19:  684,  1946. 

312.  WoRTis,  H.,  M.  H.  Stein  and  N.  Joliffe.  A. At. A.  Arch. 
Int.    Med.    69:    222,     1942. 

313.  Zander,  E.  and  G.  Weddell.  J.  Anat.  85:  68,  1951. 

314.  Zotterman,  Y.  Acta  med.  scandinav.  80:  185,   1933. 

315.  Zotterman,  Y.  J.   Physiol.  95:   i,   1939. 


CHAPTER    XX 


The  sense  of  taste 


CARL   PFAFFMANN     [     Psychology  Department,  Brown  University,  Providence,  Rhode  Island 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Receptor  Anatomy 
Neuroanatomy 
Receptor  Mechanisms 

Functional  Characteristics 
Sensitivity  and  Mechanisms  of  Stimulation 
Sour 
Salty 
Sweet 
Bitter 

Electric  taste 
Parameters  of  Stimulation 
Temperature 
Area  and  duration 
Reaction  time 
Adaptation 
Intcnsitivc  Relations 
Behavioral  Effects 


THE  SENSE  OF  TASTE,  as  distinct  from  the  other  chemo- 
ceptors,  olfaction  and  the  so-called  common  chem- 
ical sense,  is  associated  with  specialized  receptor 
organs,  the  taste  buds,  which  in  land-inhabiting  ver- 
tebrates are  located  in  the  mouth.  In  aquatic  animals 
and  insects  chemoreceptors  may  be  distributed  o\er 
the  body  surface  or  on  special  appendages  (68,  152, 
197).  In  man  taste  stimulation  is  associated  with  the 
sensation  qualities  of  salty,  sour,  bitter  and  sweet. 

Of  the  three  chemoceptors,  common  chemical 
sensitivity  is  the  least  differentiated  and  rcc|uires 
relatively  high  concentrations  for  stimulation.  Indeed, 
the  distinction  between  chemical  sensitivity  of  the 
mucous  membranes  or  moist  skin  surfaces  and  general 
pain  sensitivity  has  been  questioned  (60,  112,  161). 
Some  chemical  irritants  may  be  classed  as  lachryma- 
tories or  suffocants  (144),  depending  upon  their  sites 
of  action,  but  this  may  be  a  differentiation  largely 
because   of  the   surrounding   structures.    A   familiar 


dissociation  of  taste  and  smell  often  occurs  in  the 
temporary  anosmia  during  the  common  head  cold. 
Under  normal  circuinstances,  exclusive  stimulation 
of  taste  can  be  insured  by  placing  dilute  odorless 
solutions  on  regions  of  the  tongue  possessing  taste 
buds.  But  many  stimuli  will  activate  all  three  senses 
with  varying  degrees  of  overlap. 

The  chemical  senses  are  often  classed  among  the 
lower  sen.ses  (198)  perhaps  because  of  simplicity  of 
morphology,  relative  paucity  of  information  con- 
veyed and  relative  unimportance  in  the  sensory  life 
of  man.  Indeed,  the  loss  of  taste  is  hardly  as  incapaci- 
tating as  the  loss  of  vision  or  hearing,  at  least  to  civ- 
ilized man.  At  the  same  time,  the  chemical  senses 
mediate  such  adaptive  functions  as  food  selection  or 
the  rejection  of  noxious  stimuli,  particularly  in  the 
case  of  lower  organisms  where  dramatic  examples 
may  be  cited  (104,  177). 


RECEPTOR  AN.^TO^n• 

The  taste  buds  in  man  and  other  mammals  are 
located  primarily  on  the  edges  and  dorsum  of  the 
tongue,  and  adjacent  .surfaces  of  the  upper  margin 
of  the  gullet,  epiglottis,  soft  palate  and  pharynx  (124, 
150,  198).  On  the  tongue,  taste  buds  lie  in  the  upper 
surface  of  the  mushroom-shaped  fungiform  papillae, 
in  the  grooves  of  the  foliate  papillae,  which  are  a  set 
of  three  to  eight  folds  at  the  side  of  the  tongue  near 
the  base  and  in  the  circular  trench  of  the  vallate 
papillae  which  form  a  chevron-like  row  of  from  6  to 
15  papillae  on  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  base  of  the 
tongue  (see  fig.  i).  The  slender  keratinized  filiform 
papillae  over  most  of  the  dorsuin  contain  no  taste 
receptors.  In  certain  animals  like  the  rodents,  taste 
buds  occur  on  the  anterior  hard  palate,  especially  in 
and  around  the  nasoincisor  ducts  (i  15). 


507 


5o8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  I.  Dorsal  surface  of  the  tongue  partially  dissected  to 
show  the  nerves  to  the  posterior  part.  The  circumvallate  (C), 
fungiform  (F«)  and  filiform  (F;)  papillae  are  shown.  The 
foliate  papillae  QFo}  are  not  clearly  visible  in  this  view  since 
they  are  on  the  lateral  surface  of  the  tongue.  Taste  buds  occur 
in  C,  Fo  and  not  in  Fi.  [From  Warren  &  Carmichael  (199).] 


in  diameter  al  the  thickest  part.  In  a  wide  variety  of 
species  these  values  range  from  27  to  1 15  m  for  length 
and  14  to  70  M  for  width.  Two  kinds  of  cells  have 
been  described,  a)  the  thicker  supporting  cells  and 
ft)  the  more  slender  gustatory  cells  from  which  a  fine 
terminal  hair  projects  into  the  taste  pore,  but  these 
may  be  different  stages  in  the  age  or  functional  state 
of  but  a  single  type  (124).  During  maturity  a  con- 
tinuous process  of  atrophv  and  growth  maintains  the 
population  of  receptor  cells  at  a  relatively  stable  level. 

In  children,  taste  buds  are  more  widely  distributed 
o\er  the  hard  palate,  soft  palate,  pharyngeal  walls 
and  fungiform  papillae  of  the  middorsum  of  the 
tongue.  In  the  adult,  fungiform  papillae  are  restricted 
to  the  sides  and  edges  of  the  anterior  tongue  (198). 
Each  fungiform  papilla  contains  3  to  4  taste  buds. 
Taste  i)uds  of  the  circumvallate  papillae  show  a 
marked  atrophy  in  old  age  (i  i).  The  total  numijer  of 
taste  buds  in  man  is  probai^ly  of  the  order  of  10,000. 
It  has  been  suggested  that  in  humans  the  taste  papil- 
lae reach  full  development  at  puberty  and  remain  so 
until  the  age  of  45  when  regressixe  changes  set  in  (4). 
In  aniiuals  atrophic  changes  followed  castration  but 
could  be  reversed  by  hormone  replacement  therapy 
(5).  Such  atrophic  changes,  howe\'er,  do  not  appear 
to  diminish  taste  sensitivity  in  a  preference  test  (184; 
Warren,  R.  P.  &  C.  Pfaflfman,  unpuiilished  oi)serva- 
tions).  In  man  the  decrease  in  number  of  taste  buds 
with  age  is  correlated  with  a  decrease  in  sensitivity. 
Young  adults  recognized  sugar  solutions  at  a  lower 
mean  threshold,  0.41  per  cent  (0.012  m)  compared 
to  1.23  per  cent  (036  m)  for  elderly  subjects  (179). 

The  taste  cell  is  a  modified  epithelial  cell.  The 
taste   buds  degenerate   and   disappear  entirely   after 


FIG.  2.  Golgi  preparations  of  taste  buds  and  associated  nerve  endings.  A.  Taste  cells  and  a  'sus- 
tentive' element.  B.  Nerve  endings,  sense  cells  not  shown  (after  Retzius).  [From  Crozier  (62).] 


Taste  buds  are  goblet-shaped  clusters  of  cells 
oriented  vertically  in  the  epithelial  layer  with  a  small 
pore  opening  to  the  mucosal  surface  (fig.  2).  Human 
taste  i)uds  measure  from  60  to  80  ^i  in  length  and  40  fx 


section  of  the  taste  afferent  fibers.  When  nerve  fibers 
regenerate  to  the  periphery,  taste  buds  also  regenerate 

('49.  152)- 

Intrageminal  nerve  fibers  arise  from  a  subepithelial 


network  of  fibers  to  enter  the  taste  bud.  Here  they 
branch  a  number  of  times  to  entwine  about  and 
make  contact  with  the  surface  of  both  the  taste  and 
supporting  cells.  Two  to  three  nerve  fibers  may  enter 
each  bud  and  each  fiber  may  connect  with  one  or 
more  sense  cells.  Extrageminal  nerve  fibers  with  fine 
terminations  also  arise  from  the  same  network  of 
fibers  to  innervate  the  surrounding  epithelium  (124). 


NEURO.^NATOMY 

The  lingual  nerve  to  the  anterior  two  thirds  of  the 
tongue  subserves  touch,  temperature,  pain  and  taste. 
The  taste  aflferent  fibers  leave  this  nerve  in  a  small 
strand,  the  chorda  tympani  nerve,  which  passes 
through  the  middle  ear  cavity  close  to  the  tympanum 
to  enter  the  brain  stem  as  part  of  the  seventh  cranial 
nerve.  In  some  instances  an  alternative  pathway  via 
the  greater  superficial  petrosal  nerve  seems  indicated 
(63,  145,  188).  The  chorda  tympani  nerve  also  con- 
tains the  efTerent  fibers  for  salivation,  and  tempera- 
ture and  tactile  sensory  fibers.  The  taste  fibers  are 
moderately  small  myelinated  fibers  less  than  4  /i  in 
diameter  (70,  205).  In  the  chorda  tympani  nerve  of 
the  cat,  18  per  cent  of  the  afferent  fibers  are  unmye- 
linated (less  than  1.5  /i)  and  the  remaining  are 
myelinated,  ranging  from  1.5  to  6.0  //  in  diameter 
(77).  Taste  fibers  from  the  posterior  tongue  travel  in 
the  glossopharyngeal  and  those  from  the  larynx  and 
pharynx  in  the  vagus  (see  fig.  3). 

The  gustatory  fibers  of  the  seventh,  ninth  and 
tenth  nerves  run  into  the  tractus  solitarius  together 
with  its  nucleus  in  the  medulla.  This  tract  extends 
from  the  posterior  two  thirds  of  the  fourth  ventricle 
caudally  to  the  closed  part  of  the  medulla  where  it 
lies  dorsal  to  the  central  canal,  but  the  fibers  of  the 
seventh  and  ninth  nerves  terminate  in  the  rostral 
portion  of  the  nucleus  (172).  Insulated  wire  elec- 
trodes inserted  into  the  medulla  at  this  locus  yield 
potentials  when  chemical  stimuli  are  applied  to  the 
anterior  tongue  region  (95).  A  response  to  the  tactile 
stimulation  occurs  when  the  solution  flow  Ijegins, 
but  the  response  is  brief  compared  with  the  con- 
tinued discharge  to  taste  solutions.  Responses  from 
the  anterior  tongue  tactile  stimulation  and  anterior 
tongue  taste  stimulation  can  be  recorded  from  the 
same  electrode  loci  using  a  40  /z  insulated  wire  elec- 
trode. Taste  and  the  somatosensory  pathways  are 
closely  related  at  this  level. 

Lesions  in  the  anterior  nucleus  solitarius  produce 
degeneration    in    fibers    of    the    opposite    ascending 


Greater  superficial 
petrosal  n. 

VII  n. 

Otic  ganglion 

Petrous  ganglion 

Chorda  tympani  n. 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE  509 

.Gasserian  ganglion 

V2 

Sphenopalatine 
ganglion 

•Tongue 


FIG.  3.  The  nerve  supply  to  the  tongue.  The  solid  tines  indi- 
cate the  most  common  pathways  for  the  taste  impulses.  The 
broken  tine  indicates  an  alternative  path  from  the  chorda  tym- 
pani believed  to  exist  in  a  limited  number  of  cases.  [Modified 
from  Gushing,  1903  and  Schwartz  &  Weddell,  1938;  from 
Pfaflrmann(i6i).] 


medial  lemniscus  close  to  the  fibers  of  the  ventral 
trigeminothalamic  pathways  (7,  81).  Lesions  in  the 
region  of  the  medial  lemniscus  produced  by  a  stereo- 
taxic instrument  were  associated  with  elevated  thresh- 
olds in  a  two-bottle  preference  test  with  quinine 
solutions  (155). 

Patton  &  Ruch  following  BSrnstein  (30)  have 
emphasized  that  the  central  pathways  for  taste  are 
closely  associated  with  the  somatosensory  systems  for 
the  face  which  at  the  level  of  the  thalamus  synapse 
medially  in  the  arcuate  nucleus  (182).  Degenerating 
fibers  following  lesions  in  the  region  of  the  nucleus 
.solitarius  were  found  in  the  arcuate  nucleus  (81),  and 
retrograde  degeneration  was  noted  in  the  medial 
part  after  ablation  of  the  cortical  taste  area  (82). 
Destruction  of  a  large  portion  of  the  arcuate  nucleus 
in  the  monkey  was  followed  by  an  elevation  in  the 
quinine  preference  threshold  (157).  Unilateral  im- 
pairment of  gustatory  and  cutaneous  sensitivity  has 
been  reported  following  unilateral  tumor  of  the 
medial  part  of  the  arcuate  nucleus  opposite  to  the 
sensory  disturbance  (3).  The  failure  to  find  tactile 
representation  in  the  ventromedial  nucleus  in  spite 
of  the  fact  that  it  adjoins  the  tactile  representation  of 
the  inner  mouth  and  tongue  of  the  medial  part  of 
the  ventrobasal  complex  led  to  the  suggestion  that 
taste  as  well  as  interoceptive  fibers  may  terminate  in 
the  ventromedial  complex  (146,  182).  Monkeys  with 
ageusia  resulting  from  cortical  ablation  showed  lesions 
in  the  \'entromedial  complex  (13).  Taste-sparing 
cortical  lesions  of  the  inferior  Rolandic  cortex  are 


iio 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


associated  with  severe  degeneration  throug;hout  the 
arcuate  nucleus  except  for  the  dorsomedial  tip  (155). 

The  close  association  of  taste  pathways  with  the 
somatosensory  and  also  motor  mechanisms  appears 
to  hold  true  at  the  cortical  level.  Bremer  (31)  showed 
that  ablation  of  the  masticatory  cortex  in  rabbits  is 
associated  with  a  taste  deficit.  Changes  in  the  elec- 
trocorticogram  from  this  same  area  were  observed  in 
unanesthetized  rabbits  when  quinine  solutions  were 
placed  on  the  tongue  (72).  A  corresponding  area  on 
the  orbital  surfaces  of  the  rat  ijrain  (25)  was  identi- 
fied by  the  e\oked  potential  method  following  elec- 
trical stimulation  of  the  chorda  tympani  and  glosso- 
pharyngeal nerves.  This  corresponds  to  the  region 
from  which  masticatory  movements  could  be  elicited 
by  electrical  stimulation  (130)  and  thus  is  a  sensory 
motor  area.  Ablation  of  this  area  in  the  rat  led  to  an 
elevation  of  the  two-bottle  preference  thresholds  for 
quinine  .solutions.  No  taste  deficits  were  noted  in  two 
animals  in  which  most  of  the  neocortex  except  for  the 
combined  chorda  tympani  and  ninth  nerve  receiving 
areas  was  removed.  Further  studies  of  the  deficits 
produced  by  cortical  aijlation  indicate  that  under 
certain  high  drive  states  no  apparent  taste  deficit 
can  be  demonstrated.  A  thirsty  normal  animal  and 
a  thirsty  animal  with  a  cortical  lesion  will  show  the 
same  aversion  to  quinine.  Both  have  higher  thresholds 
for  quinine  than  the  normal  animal  with  water  pres- 
ent ad  libitum.  Thus,  the  removal  of  the  cortical 
taste  area  does  not  make  the  animal  ageusic  but  in- 
stead renders  the  animal  less  discriminating  in  an  ad 
liijitum  situation  (23,  24). 

The  chorda  tympani  nerve  area  of  the  rat  and  cat 
lies  in  the  face  somatic  area  (156,  201).  Much  of  the 
surface-positive  cortical  response  to  electrical  stimu- 
lation of  the  chorda  tympani  is  due  to  the  tactile  af- 
ferent fibers  in  that  nerve.  It  has  not  been  possible  to 
record  an  evoked  cortical  potential  with  gross  sur- 
face electrodes  with  adequate  taste  stimulation.  How- 
ever, ultramicroelectrodc  probings  in  the  tactile 
tongue  area  of  the  cat  did  yield  single  units  that  re- 
sponded to  taste  but  not  to  touch  or  temperature. 
Other  units  in  this  area  showed  convergence  of  tactile, 
thermal  and  gustatory  impulses  (128).  The  taste  units 
appeared  to  be  less  chemically  specific  than  the  single 
afferent  fibers  for  they  responded  to  almost  all  types 
of  gustatory  stimulation  Cso)- 

In  monkeys  and  chimpanzees,  lesions  of  the  face 
motor  and  sensory  areas  along  the  free  surface  of  the 
lower  Rolandic  cortex  did  not  produce  taste  deficits 
in  preference  tests  (155).  Taste  deficits  occurred  only 
when  the  lesions  involved  the  buried  opercular  and 


parainsular  cortex.  Bagshaw  &  Pribram  (13)  have 
shown  that  the  insular  and  anterior  supratemporal 
as  well  as  parainsular  cortex  all  must  lie  included  to 
lead  to  an  elevation  of  threshold.  Some  elevation  of 
threshold  followed  ablation  of  the  operculum  plus 
insula,  but  not  with  restricted  ablation  of  the  insula 
or  insula  and  anterior  supratemporal  plane. 

In  man,  a  series  of  patients  with  bullet  wounds  of 
the  inferior  postcentral  region  showed  reduced  gusta- 
tory and  tactual  sensibility  of  the  tongue  (30).  Pen- 
field  &  Boldrey  (159)  elicited  gustatory  sensations  in 
conscious  human  patients  by  electrical  stimulation 
of  the  lower  end  of  the  postcentral  gyrus.  Thus,  the 
evidence  implicates  the  region  of  the  cortex  of  the 
operculum,  insula  and  supratemporal  plane  of  the 
temporal  lobe. 

Patton  (155)  notes  that  not  only  is  there  the  close 
approximation  of  the  taste  to  the  somatosensory 
system  but  that  taste  localization  fits  into  its  orderly 
topographical  arrangement.  Taste  does  not  have  a 
special  primary  cortical  receiving  zone  with  exclusive 
gustatory  functions. 


REGEPTOR    MECHANISMS 

Functional  Characteristics 

No  simple  relation  can  be  established  between 
chemical  stimuli  and  taste  quality  except  perhaps  in 
the  case  of  acid.  Equally  sour  concentrations  of 
hydrochloric,  sulfuric,  nitric,  phosphoric  and  acetic 
acids  are  said  to  be  indistinguishable  from  each  other 
when  odor  is  excluded  (56);  but  sucrose,  dextrose 
and  lactose  do  not  ha\e  exactly  the  same  taste  (44); 
and  stimuli  that  elicit  the  bitter  taste  can  be  dis- 
criminated from  each  other.  The  taste  qualities  of  in- 
organic salts  are  complex  and  only  sodium  chloride 
has  a  pure  saline  taste,  yet  in  threshold  solutions  this 
is  variously  reported  as  .sweet  or  bitter  (173,  181). 

The  tongue  surface  is  not  uniformly  sensitive  to 
punctate  stimulation.  The  middorsum  is  insensitive 
to  all  tastes.  Sweet  sen.sitivity  is  greatest  at  the  tip, 
sour  at  the  sides,  bitter  at  the  jjack,  while  salt  sen.si- 
tivity is  relatively  homogeneous  but  greatest  at  the 
tip  (96).  Indi\idual  papillae  have  been  found  to 
react  exclusively  to  salt,  to  sweet  or  to  sour,  or  to 
come  combination  of  two,  three  or  four  of  the  basic 
taste  stimuli  (120).  Certain  drugs  have  a  differential 
effect  on  sensitivity.  Gymnemic  acid,  an  extract  of 
the  leaves  of  an  Indian  plant  Gynmema  sylvestre,  reduces 
sensitivitv  for  sweet  and  bitter  but  leaves  salt  and  sour 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


relatively  uninfluenced  (191).  Such  observations  led 
to  the  view  that  taste  consisted  of  four  different 
modalities,  salt,  sour,  bitter  and  sweet,  each  with  its 
particular  type  of  receptor  even  though  no  obvious 
histological  diflferences  distinguished  taste  buds  from 
different  regions  of  the  tongue  (147). 

Electrophysiological  studies  show  that  the  chorda 
tympani  nerve  discharge  elicited  by  taste  solutions 
on  the  tongue,  varies  with  concentration  above  the 
threshold.  In  figure  4  the  basic  taste  stimuli  can  be 
ranked  in  order  of  effectiveness:  quinine,  hydro- 
chloric acid,  sodium  chloride,  potassium  chloride  and 


400. 


300  _ 


ZOO. 


3  -2 

LOG    MOLAR    CONG 


FIG.  4.  Height  of  integrator  deflections  to  stimuli  of  differ- 
ent concentrations  in  one  cat  preparation.  Ordinate  gives  de- 
flections in  arbitrary  units.  [From  Pfaffmann  (164).] 


sucrose  but  the  exact  order  or  magnitude  of  response 
\aries  with  species.  In  rodents  (rat,  guinea  pig  and 
hamster)  sodium  chloride  is  more  effective  than 
potassium  chloride,  whereas  in  carnivores  (raccoon, 
cat  and  dog)  the  converse  is  true.  There  is  very  little 
response  to  sugar  in  the  cat,  somewhat  more  in  the 
rat  and  still  more  in  the  hamster  and  guinea  pig.  The 
quinine  response  is  better  in  the  cat  than  in  the  rat 
or  rabbit  (21,  163). 

A  typical  single  fiber  discharge  from  the  rat  chorda 
tympani  is  shown  in  figure  5  (164).  The  threshold 
varies  from  unit  to  unit  so  that  as  concentration  in- 
creases there  is  an  increase  both  in  the  number  of 
units  active  and  in  the  frequency  of  discharge.  The 
fiber  in  figure  5  also  responded  to  hydrochloric  acid 
and  potassium  chloride.  A  wider  sample  of  the  'spec- 
trum' of  sensibilities  of  different  fibers  in  the  rat 
chorda  tympani  is  shown  in  figure  6.  The  pattern  of 
sensitivity  varies  from  fiber  to  fiber.  Although  some 
elements  (.4  and  fi)  are  relatively  specific,  others 
(especially  /)  have  a  broader  sensitivity.  These  dif- 
ferent sensitivities  cannot  be  readily  grouped  into  the 
basic  four  types  of  classical  theory.  Studies  (121)  in 
which  micropipettes  have  been  inserted  into  the 
individual  cells  of  the  taste  bud  show  the  same  kind 
of  sensitivity  pattern  in  the  receptor  cells  thernseh-es. 
This  important  observation  disposes  of  the  possi- 
bility that  the  overlapping  sensitivities  of  single  fibers 
of  the  chorda  tympani  result  from  the  branching  of 
fibers  and  termination  upon  more  than  one  type  o 


1.0 


-liiM—L 


•   r»>-ii'iiM>        n   I'lr^i'iiO       '  i~  Wi 1J._.    _i,».|iii 


■C'^»»<^ 


rJiiiiiiL.liilJl.l..Jl.J,.>i 


ni^^f^^^Wv* 


J,,.L.i.i,,  I  ...UlLiLL  L. 


iirQii    -     If^  ill  UililJ.L  [1    liii  ■■|J--1-      ^  J,      \^--i      I        -I'll*       '|-1|-|     ■      -I      -     I        -      -\      i.'~\\ 


.03 


^^"^^^■^^^W^ri^iiJ^  LJ*^^— V  ■^^^'W'U^^  ^ 


*pJ-i  ^.LLLL l'y*.»*>*  ^u.^^  L  .1  ■   ■'  / 


l^i^«i  tft^^if^t^  ^i.>^iy»>^>»>iOx^^»J>vt^|^»W>i>^>«iX^ 


.01 


.003 


4«M^^B«i^i»4irflMM 


MMMtavNwl^«Mb«N 


.001 


inpi|i->^ii  i<liiU>> 


HzO 


RAT        No  CI    Series 


I I 

.1  sec. 


FIG.  5.  Response  of  a  single  element  from  the  rat  sensitive  to  NaCl.  This  element  also  responds 
to  HCl  and  KCl.  Responses  to  quinine  and  sucrose  were  insignificant.  [From  Pfaffmann  (164).] 


512  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^^  NEUROPHVSIOLOGV 


FREQ 
40- 


RAT 

SINGLE      ELEMENTS 


(01) 


M 

1 

(3) 

03N 

.IM      IM 

OIM 

1.0  M 

HCI 

KCI   NaCI 

Qu 

Sue 

.03N 
HCI 


IM 
KCI 


.1  M 
NoCI 


OIM 
Q  u. 


1.0  M 
Sue. 


03N 
H  CI 


IM 
KCI 


.1  M 
NaCI 


OIM    lOM 
Qu.     Sue. 


FIG.  6.  Bar  graphs  summarizing  frequency  of  response  during  the  first  second  to  fi\'e  standard 
taste  solutions  in  nine  different  single  fiber  preparations  in  the  rat.  Sucrose  of  0.3  m  was  used  as 
test  solution  in  eleinents  D  and  /,  0.01  M  HCI  in  element  /.  In  all  other  cases  concentrations  are 
as  shown  on  abscissa.  Cross-hatehrd  bar  graph  superimposed  on  figure  for  element  E  shows  relative 
magnitude  of  integrator  response  for  test  solutions.  Figures  in  parentheses  give  magnitudes  in  arbi- 
trary units.  Note  that  only  elements  D  and  G  resemble  the  response  of  the  total  nerve.  [From  Pfaff- 
mann  (164).] 


receptor  cell.  The  proijlcm  of  chemical  specificity  is 
one  of  specificity  within  the  individual  cells,  i.e.  to 
different  sites  or  loci  on  the  cell  membrane. 

The  specificity  of  the  receptor  unit  cannot  be  ade- 
quately described  by  the  response  to  only  one  con- 
centration of  a  test  stimulus.  Figure  7  shows  that 
fiber  B  (the  same  as  fiber  B  in  fig.  6)  can  be  stimu- 
lated by  sodium  chloride  at  concentrations  of  o.  i  m 
and  higher.  This  might  be  labelled  as  a  salt-sugar 
unit.    Since   gymnemic   acid   applied   to   the   tongue 


leads  to  a  clear-cut  decrement  in  the  response  to  sugar 
with  no  change  for  sodium  chloride,  it  appears  that 
only  the  sucrose  sites  on  the  cell  is  blocked,  not  the 
'salt-sucrose'  cell  itself.  The  differential  suppression  of 
sugar  sensitivity,  often  cited  as  evidence  for  separate 
modalities  of  taste,  can  be  equally  well  encompassed 
by  a  theory  of  specific  sites  on  the  cell  membrane. 

The  two  fibers,  A  and  B  of  figure  7,  respond  to 
ijoth  sodium  chloride  and  sucrose,  but  A  is  more 
reactive  to  sodium  chloride  and  B  is  more  reactive  to 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


513 


n 

N.C,        f^\ 

sue       ^0 

30. 

1 
1 
1 
1 

A 

1            T 
/              1 

B                      /                      / 

JO. 

/ 

1 

/                     / 
/                    / 

■0. 

1 

/ 

/ 
/ 

/ 
/ 

sue. 

/, 

L^ 

/                   / 
/                > 

/                                  / 

6               ^^      HoCl 

- 

—* 1 1 

S                 -2                  -1 

0            1 
0 

_2 

-1                      0 

TABLE  I .  Fiber  Type  Response"* 


FIG.  7.  Graph  comparing  relative  specificities  of  two  dif- 
ferent elements  in  rat.  Each  is  sensitive  to  NaCl  and  sucrose 
(as  well  as  other  stimuli).  Element  A  is  relatively  more  sensitive 
to  NaCl;  B  is  relatively  more  sensitive  to  sucrose.  Ordinate 
gives  frequency  in  the  first  second  of  discharge.  [From  Pfaff- 
mann  (164).] 


sucrose.  At  all  concentrations  of  sodium  chloride,  the 
frequencies  in  .-1  are  higher  than  that  in  B;  at  all 
concentrations  of  sucrose,  B  is  greater  than  A.  .Such 
a  two-fiber  system,  therefore,  signals  sodium  chloride 
when  A  is  greater  than  B  and  sucrose  when  B  is 
greater  than  A. 

Thus  different  information  may  be  convened  by  the 
same  nerve  fiber  depending  upon  the  activity  in  a 
second  parallel  afferent  fiber.  From  figure  7  it  can  be 
seen  that  a  discharge  of  6  impulses  in  B  with  no  ac- 
tivity of  .-1  is  correlated  with,  that  is,  signals  .05  m 
sucrose.  A  discharge  of  6  impulses  in  B  plus  a  dis- 
charge of  32  impulses  in  A  is  correlated  with  or 
signals  o.  i  m  sodium  chloride.  Intensity  would  be 
correlated  with  an  increase  in  overall  frequency  of 
discharge. 

Such  a  model  may  be  expanded  by  adding  more 
fibers  to  provide  a  greater  variety  of  combinations  of 
discharge  pattern.  If  sensory  quality  depends  upon 
such  patterns,  we  might  expect  quality  of  sensation 
to  change  as  the  afferent  population  is  reduced,  for 
example  when  the  stimulus  concentration  approaches 
threshold.  Such  changes  in  quality  are  well  known 
(see  table  3). 

The  further  discovery  that  in  certain  species  water 
alone  leads  to  an  increase  in  afferent  acti\ity  (136, 
164,  206)  provides  still  another  dimension  by  which 
discrimination  can  be  mediated.  Thus,  a  decrease  in 
stimulus  concentration  will  be  as.sociated  with  an  in- 
crease in  afferent  activity.  The  base  line  or  'zero' 
taste,   therefore,  is  not  provided  by  pure  water  but 


Stimulus 

'Water' 
Fiber 

'Salt' 
Fiber 

'Acid' 
Fiber 

'Qui- 
nine' 
Fiber 

Sensation 
Evoked 

H;0  (salt  <o.03  m) 

+ 

0 

0 

0 

-^ 

water 

NaCl  (0.05  m) 

0 

+ 

0 

0 

-^ 

salt 

HCl  (pH  2.5) 

+ 

+ 

-1- 

0 

-. 

sour 

Quinine 

+ 

0 

0 

+ 

— > 

bitter 

*  According  to  Cohen  et  al.  (49). 


perhaps  by  the  natural  environment  of  saliva.  Cohen 
el  al.  have  elaborated  the  pattern  concept  with  a 
schema  (shown  in  table  i)  incorporating  the  water 
receptor  (49).  This  elaborates  the  earlier  model  de- 
scribed by  Pfaffmann  (160),  but  it  is  not  clear  that 
such  'typing'  best  describes  the  taste  receptor  spectrum 
(75).  Increasing  concentrations,  for  example,  may 
bring  in  other  stimuli  so  that  a  wider  sampling  of 
stimuli  might  change  the  'types.' 

It  is  clear  from  recent  electrophysiological  evidence 
that  the  taste  receptors  do  not  always  fall  into  four 
basic  receptor  types  corresponding  to  the  basic  taste 
qualities.  The  individual  sensory  cells  are  differen- 
tially sensitive  to  chemicals,  probably  because  of  dif- 
ferences at  sites  on  the  cell  membrane.  The  chemical 
specificity  of  the  taste  cell  can  best  be  described  as  a 
cluster  of  sensitivities  which  varies  among  different 
receptor  cells.  Any  one  cell  is  reactive  to  a  varving 
degree  to  a  number  of  different  chemical  stimuli, 
many  of  which  fall  in  two  or  more  of  the  four  classical 
basic  taste  categories. 


Sensitivity  and  Mechanisms  of  Stimulation 

SOUR.  It  has  long  been  known  that  the  sour  taste  is 
associated  with  the  hydrogen  ion  and  that,  in  a  rough 
way,  the  degree  of  sourness  is  related  to  the  degree  of 
dissociation.  Strong  acids  (fully  dissociated)  are  more 
sour  than  equinormal  solutions  of  a  weak  acid  like 
acetic  (114,  174)-  Neutralizing  the  acid  eliminates 
the  sour  taste,  but  not  all  acids  are  sour.  Amino  acids 
are  sweet  and  picric  acid  is  intensely  bitter. 

Inspection  of  taste  threshold  data  often  reveals  wide 
discrepancies  from  one  investigator  to  another.  Table 
2  summarizes  selected  data  on  the  acid  lower  thresh- 
olds. The  range  of  \ariation  among  different  refer- 
ences cited  is  shown,  such  variation  being  probably 
partly  the  result  of  valid  individual  differences  among 
subjects  and  partly  the  result  of  differences  in  meth- 
ods of  determining  thresholds.  Threshold  may  be 
given  as  a  sensitivity  measure,  i.e.  the  minimum  con- 


5'4 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


TABLE  2.  Acid  Thresholds  in  Man  {in  .Normal  Concentrations) 


Substance 

Formula 

Mol. 

Wt. 

Median 

Range 

n 

Ref.« 

Hydrochloric 

HCl 

36-5 

.0009 

.00005-. 01 

(.0) 

a,  c, 

b.g 

Nitric 

HNO. 

63 

.001 1 

.001-. 0063 

(4) 

g 

Sulfuric 

H2SO, 

98 

.001 

. 00005- • °02 

(5) 

a,  c, 

g 

Formic 

HCOOH 

46 

0 

.0018 

. 0007- . 0035 

(3) 

f.g 

Acetic 

CH3COOH 

60 

.0018 

.0001-. 0058 

(9) 

a,  c, 

b,  d,  e,  f 

Butyric 

CH3(CH0:COOH 

88 

.0020 

. 0005- . 0035 

(2) 

f.g 

Oxalic 

COOHCOOH-aH.O 

126 

.  0026 

.0020-. 0032 

(2) 

f.  g 

Succinic 

COOH(CH,)iCOOH 

118 

.0032 

.0016-. 0094 

(3) 

f,g 

Lactic 

CH.CHOHCOOH 

90 

.0016 

.00052-. 0028 

(4) 

b,  f, 

g 

Malic 

HOOCCH(OH)CH..C:OOH 

'34 

.0016 

.001 3-. 0023 

(3) 

b,  g 

Tartaric 

HOOC(CHOH),COOH 

H,0 

168 

.0012 

.000025-. 0072 

(8) 

a,  b 

d,  e,  f,  g 

Citric 

(COOH)CH;C(OH)(COOH)CH2COOH 

192 

.0023 

.00 1 3-. 0057 

(4) 

b,  g 

f.g 


This  table  is  based  on  values  cited  in  von  Skramlik  C198)  and  certain  more  recent  studies.  Data  from  earlier  literature  in 
other  compilations  were  not  incorporated  because  of  frequent  errors  of  computation  observed  therein  or  uncertainties  of 
method  or  technique  of  e.xperimentation.  Values  shown  are  the  median  of  several  values,  the  number  being  shown  in  column 
n.  The  ranges  of  values  are  also  reported.  Hahn's  (90)  values  of  .003  .V  for  all  acids  (except  butyric  and  malic  acids)  were 
not  included  in  this  table. 

*  a,  Cragg  (59);  b,  Fabian  &  Blum  (74);  c,  Gibson  &  Hartmann  (83),  d,  Hopkins  (iio);  e,  Knowles  et  at.  (123);  f,  Taylor 
(195);  g,  (Paul  and  Bohnen,  Corin,  Richards,  Heymann,  Richet,  Renqvist)  cited  by  von  Skramlik  (198). 


centration  at  which  a  difference  from  water  can  be 
detected  or  as  a  recognition  threshold,  i.e.  where  the 
quahty  can  be  recognized.  The  former  are  usually 
lower. 

Weak  organic  acids  appear  more  sovir  than  would 
be  expected  from  their  degree  of  dissociation.  At 
threshold,  the  hydrogen  ion  concentration  of  acetic 
acid  is  less  than  that  of  hydrochloric  acid.  Liljestrand 
(135)  found  that  the  pH  of  weak  organic  acids  at 
threshold  ranged  from  3.7  to  3.9,  for  strong  mineral 
acids  from  3.4  to  3.5.  The  findings  of  equal  pH  (ap- 
pro.ximately  4.4)  for  organic  and  inorganic  acids  (59) 
or  equal  normality  of  .003  n  for  all  acids  (89)  are  at 
variance  with  the  more  common  result  of  different 
pH  and  different  normalities  at  threshold  (12,  26, 
114,  158,  174,  195). 

Cragg  C58)  noted  that  subjects  with  a  more  alkaline 
saliv'a  required  more  concentrated  hydrochloric  acid 
solutions  to  match  an  acetic  acid  standard.  The  sour 
taste  of  buffers  and  of  solutions  of  the  monobasic 
salts  of  organic  acids  can  be  detected  at  pH  values 
which  are  lower  than  those  of  inorganic  acid  solutions 
(12,  135,  158}.  Buffer  solutions  held  in  the  mouth 
retain  the  sour  taste  longer  than  does  plain  acid.  The 
pH  of  acetic  acid  changes  less  than  the  pH  of  hydro- 
chloric acid  after  being  held  in  the  mouth. 

The  relative  stimulating  efficiency  of  suprathresh- 
old  concentrations  studied  by  means  of  equal  sourness 
matches  between  different  acids  and  the  standard, 
hydrochloric  acid,  is  shown  in  figure  8.  On  the  basis 
of   hydrogen    ion    concentration,    the    organic    acids 


acetic,  carbonic,  tartaric,  lactic  and  acetylactic  acids 
were  all  more  sour  than  hvdrochloric  acid  (16,  103, 

158). 

These  effects  are  not  clue  solely  to  the  buffering 
action  of  saliva.  When  acid  solutions  are  applied  by 
a  flow  system  applicator  so  that  the  saliva  is 
thoroughly  rinsed  off,  equal  afferent  nerve  discharge 
was  not  achieved  with  equal  pH,  ecjual  normality  or 
molarity  (20).  Figure  9  shows  the  magnitude  of 
response  in  the  chorda  tympani  discharge  produced 
by  different  acids  of  the  same  pH. 

Thus,  some  basic  physiological  mechanism  com- 
plicates the  relation  between  sourness  and  acidity. 
Richards  (174)  suggested  that  the  hydrogen  ions 
might  react  with  some  substance  on  the  receptor 
surface  so  that,  as  these  ions  in  a  solution  of  the  organic 
acid  were  taken  up,  further  dissociation  would  re- 
place them.  Others  (103)  refer  to  the  potential  as  well 
as  actual  hydrogen  ion  concentration  as  a  deter- 
miner of  sourness.  Kenrick  (116)  and  Beatty  &  Cragg 
(16)  noted  that  the  amount  of  phosphate  buffer 
(pH  7)  necessary  to  bring  equisour  concentrations  of 
different  acids  to  a  pH  4.4  was  proportional  to  the 
sourness  defined  by  the  normality  of  an  equisour 
hydrochloric  acid  solution.  Ostwald  &  Kuhn  (151) 
noted  a  parallel  between  the  sourness  and  the  swell- 
ing of  gelatin  in  different  acids.  Sourness  has  been 
attributed  to  the  rate  at  which  the  acid  penetrates 
the  cell  or  intracellular  spaces  (61)  or  to  adsorption 
on  the  cell  surface  (195)- 

Acid  stimulation  of  the  integument  of  lower  organ- 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


515 


/        2       3 
Homentrvtion  der  SHuren. 


5       6        7 

Mi  III  mo/  in  II 


[HJCHiCOOH 

(H-)COOHCH(OH)CII(0/I)COOH 
fl^Hi  C03> 1— I 


FIG.  8.  Equal  sourness  matches.  Ordmates  give  concentrations  of  HC:l  required  to  match  the  sour- 
ness of  other  acids  at  concentrations  shown  on  the  abscissae.  Broken  lines  give  hydrogen  ion  concen- 
trations of  weak  acid  and  salts.  [From  Paul-Munchen  (158).] 


FIG.  9.  Integrated  response  of  the  rat  chorda  tympani  nerve  to  hydrochloric,  citric,  formic,  ox- 
alic, acetic  and  hydrochloric  acids  ^reading  from  left  to  rig/it')  at  pH  2.5.  Duration  of  response,  10  to 
20  sec.  [From  Beidler  (20).] 


5i6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


TABLE  3.  The  Taste  of  Salts  at  Different  Concentrations* 


X 
a. 


NUMBER    OF   C    ATOMS 


-t- 


FIG.  10.  pH  required  to  elicit  a  constant  reaction  time  in 
the  sunfish,  for  hydrochloric  acid  and  a  series  of  normal  ali- 
phatic acids  of  increasing  carbon  chain  length.  [From  ."Mlison 
(8).] 


isms  (fish,  frogs,  barnacles,  mollusks,  worms)  is 
probably  due  to  the  general  sensitivity  and  thus 
simpler  than  in  the  case  of  taste.  Stimulation  by 
mineral  acids  in  this  case  is  determined  by  pH,  but 
normal  aliphatic  acids  stimulate  at  lower  hydrogen 
ion  concentrations,  the  efficacy  increasing  systemat- 
ically with  increasing  chain  length  (8,  g,  54,  55) 
(see  fig.  10).  Similarly,  the  nonpolar  parts  of  the 
molecule  in  a  series  of  n  aliphatic  alcohols  add  to 
stimulating  efficiency  with  increasing  number  of 
carbon  atoms  (53).  The  same  holds  for  a  series  of 
alcohols  and  glycols  for  taste  in  man  and  in  insect 
chemoception  (66).  In  the  insect  studies  a  wider 
sampling  of  organic  molecules  was  employed.  Stimu- 
lation appeared  to  be  associated  with  increasing 
lipoid  solubility  attendant  upon  increased  chain 
length  and  the  introduction  of  functional  groups  that 
reduced  water  solubility  (65,  68,  6g).  The  hydrogen 
ion  is  clearly  an  important  determinant  of  the  sour- 
ness of  acids,  but  it  alone  does  not  determine  stimu- 
lating efficiency. 

s.\LTY.  All  substances  with  a  s<ilty  taste  are  soluble 
salts  composed  of  positive  and  negative  ions  in  the 
solid  (crystalline)  state  which  dissolve  in  water  to 
Droduce  a  solution  of  these  ions.  Sodium  chloride  is 


M 

NaCl 

KCl 

0.009 

no  taste 

sweet 

O.OIO 

weak  sweet 

strong  sweet 

0.0  J 

sweet 

sweet,  perhaps  bitter 

0.03 

sweet 

bitter 

0.04 

salt,  slightly  sweet 

bitter 

0.05 

salty 

bitter,  salty 

0.  I 

salty 

bitter,  salty 

0.2 

pure  salty 

salty,  bitter,  sour 

I  .0 

pure  salty 

salty,  bitter,  sour 

*  From  R 

engvist  (173). 

the  only  substance  said  to  possess  the  'pure  sahy  taste' 
except  that  the  threshold  concentrations  of  this  salt 
taste  sweet  (see  taljle  3).  Other  salts  display  the  same 
phenomenon  but  yield  complex  salty  tastes  at  supra- 
thresholcl  values. 

Both  the  anion  and  cation  contribute  to  the  taste 
quality  and  to  the  stimulating  efficiency  (80).  Thus, 
whereas  .04  m  sodium  chloride  is  distinctly  salty, 
sodium  acetate  of  the  same  concentration  has  no 
salty  taste.  In  a  series  of  sodium  salts,  the  quality  of 
the  taste  elicited  will  vary  with  the  anion.  A  similar 
effect  can  be  noted  in  a  chloride  series  with  different 
cations.  In  a  series  of  halides  of  the  monovalent 
alkali  metals  (plus  ammonium)  the  low  molecular 
weight  (below  iio)  salts  are  predominantly  salty  in 
taste,  while  the  higher  molecular  weight  (over  160) 
salts  are  bitter  (122).  Salts  of  heavy  metals  such  as 
mercury  have  a  metallic  taste  but  some  lead  salts, 
especially  lead  acetate  (sugar  of  lead),  and  beryllium 
salts  are  sweet. 

The  thresholds  for  different  salts  ha\c  been  vari- 
ously reported  to  be  equimolar  for  the  cation  (92), 
for  halogen  salts  (84),  inversely  related  to  the  molecu- 
lar weight  (80),  directly  related  to  cation  mobility 
(79).  Table  4  shows  the  median  values  ba.sed  on  a 
sampling  of  a  numljer  of  different  threshold  studies  in 
man. 

von  Skramlik  (198)  attempted  to  specify  objectively 
the  complex  taste  of  salts  by  means  of  the  following 
taste  equation :  .'Y  =  .v.^  -|-  yB  -\-  zC  -\-  vD  in  which 


>■' 


and  v  are  molar  concentration  values  and  .-1 


stands  for  sodium  chloride;  B,  quinine  sulphate;  C, 
fructose;  D,  potassium  tartrate;  and  .A'  is  the  molar 
concentration  of  the  salt  being  matched.  Although 
indi\idual  differences  among  subjects  are  clearly 
apparent  in  the  matches,  certain  trends  or  consist- 
encies can  be  noted. 

The  degree  of  saltiness  of  a  series  of  salts  is  given 
b>  the  ratio  of  m  NaCl/M  'salt'  required  to  match  the 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


5'7 


TABLE  4.  Salt  Thresholds  in  Man  (in  Molar  Concentrations) 


Substance 
Lithium  chloride 
Ammonium  chloride 
Sodium  chloride 


Potassium  chloride 
Magnesiiun  chloride 
Calcium  chloride 
Sodium  fluoride 
Sodium  bromide 
Sodium  iodide 


Formula 
LiCl 
NH4CI 
NaCl 


Mol.  VVt. 
42.4 

53-5 
58.5 


Median 
025 
004" 
01'' 

oy 

017 
015" 


KCl  74.6 

MgCb  95.23 

CaClj  110-99 

NaF  42 .  00 

NaBr  102.91 

Nal  14992 

*  a,  Cox  &  Nathans  (57);  b,  Fabian  &  Blum  (74);  c,  Frings  (79) 
et  al.  (123):  g,  Richter  &  MacLean  (181);  h,  von  Skramlik  (198):  i,  Hober  &  Kiesow  (106). 
»  Mean  value.         ''  Sensitivity  threshold.         '  Recognition  threshold. 

'^  Hahn  (92)  has  reported  one  subject  with  a  threshold  for  NaCl  of  less  than  13  X   io~'m.  This  is  difficult  to 
value  is  far  beyond  the  values  commonly  reported. 


Range 
. 009- . 04 
.001-. 009 
. 00 I - . 08 
■OO3-.085 
.001 -.07 
.003-. 04 
. 002- . 03 
.001-. 04 
.008-. 04 
. C04- . I 


3 
8 
10 
6 
3 

1 

2 
3 


d,  e,  g<i 
g.  h 


h,  i 


005 
024 
028 
;  d,  Hopkins  (no);  e,  Janowitz  &  Grossman  (in),  1,  Knowlcs 

terpret  for  the 


TABLE  5.  Mean  Salt  Qjioticnt  (M  NaCl/M  'Salt') 
for  Different  Salts* 


NH, 

K 

Ca 

Na 
Li 

Mg 


CI 
2.83 
1.36 
1.23 
1 .00 
0.44 
0.20 


2.44 
0.54 

0.77 
0.57 


Br 
..83 
I  .16 

0.91 
0.79 


SO, 
1.26 
0.26 

1-25 
o.oi 


NOi 
1.03 
o.  14 

0.17 
0.23 


HCOi 


0.23 


Quotients  show  the  molar  concentration  of  NaCl  required 
to  match  the  saltiness  of  the  comparison  salt. 
*  From  von  Skramlik  (198). 


saltiness  ignoring  all  other  components.  Table  5 
showing  the  mean  values  for  each  of  several  series  of 
salts  gives  the  following  cation  series  in  the  case  of  the 
chlorides:  NH4  >  K  >  Ca  >  Na  >  Li  >  Mg. 
This  closely  resembles  the  series  found  (79)  in  a 
comparative  study  of  rejection  thresholds  in  animals 
and  detection  thresholds  in  man.  Frings'  (79)  attempt 
to  relate  a  single  property,  cation  mobility,  to  the 
stimulating  efficiency  of  electrolytes  across  all  species 
is  premature  in  view  of  the  demonstrated  species 
differences  in  sensitivity  based  on  electrophysiological 
study  (21,  163).  The  typical  series  for  carnivores; 
NH4  >  Ca  >  Sr  >  K  >  Mg  >  Na  >  Li  may  be 
contrasted  with  that  for  the  rat;  Li  >  Na  >  NH4  > 
Ca  >  K  >  Sr  >  Mg  which  is  typical  of  the  rodent 
cla.ss.  The  differences  in  the  relation  of  sodium  to 
potassium  in  the  two  orders  is  striking.  Sodium  is 
very  effective  in  rodents  but  relatively  ineffective  in 
carnivores,  whereas  potassium  is  relatively  ineffective 
in  both.  Beidler  ?<  al.  (21)  have  pointed  out  that  the 
sodium/potassium  ratios  in  the  red   blood  cells  are 


high  (16. 1 )  for  carnivores  and  low-  (0.12)  for  rodents, 
perhaps  indicati\'e  of  a  species  difference  in  the 
physicochemical  make  up  of  the  membranes  of  the 
receptors.  The  seriation  NH3  >  K  >  Na  >  Li  found 
in  the  withdrawal  reaction  of  the  frog  and  other 
lower  arjuatic  forms  when  these  solutions  are  applied 
to  the  integument  is  probably  due  to  the  less  dif- 
ferentiated   common   chemical    sensitivity   (52,    109, 

153)- 

The  anion  series  based  on  tabic  5  for  sodium  salts 

is  SO4  >  CI  >  Br  >  I  >  HCO3  >  NO:,.  Among 
invertebrates  the  following  seriation  has  been  de- 
scribed; I  >  Br  >  NO3  >  CI.  In  the  chorda  tympani 
of  the  rat,  the  anion  has  a  much  smaller  effect  than 
the  cation  but  the  following  series  can  be  noted: 
CI  =  Br  >  NO;,  >  citrate  >  SO,  >  CO.,  (18). 

Such  ionic  .seriations  (variously  called  Hofmeister 
series,  lyotropic  series,  etc.)  can  be  demonstrated  in  a 
number  of  other  phenomena  as  the  penetration  into 
cells  or  adsorption  on  surfaces  (105).  Beidler  (19)  has 
recently  developed  a  theory  which  provides  some  basis 
for  choosing  among  these  possibilities.  His  basic 
taste  equation  is: 


C 
R 


Rt„  IlRn 


where  C  equals  the  concentration  of  the  stimulus,  R 
is  the  response  magnitude  of  chorda  tympani  dis- 
charge, Rm  is  the  maximal  response  magnitude  and 
A'  is  an  equilibrium  constant.  A  plot  of  C/R  against 
C  yields  a  straight  line  with  a  slope  equal  to  i/Rm 
and  a  y  intercept  equal  to  i/K'Rm-  The  equation  is 
similar  to  Langmuir's  adsorption  isotherm  and  to  the 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


o.e 


0.7- 


0.6- 


0.5- 


0.4- 


0.1- 


0.2 


0.4 


0.6 


0.8 


1.0 


FIG.   II.  Ratio  of  molar  concentration  and  magnitude  of  integrated  cliorda  tympani  response 
plotted  against  molar  concentration  of  stimulus.  Explanation  in  text.  [From  Beidler  (19).] 


equation  expressing  the  i^inding  of  ions  by  proteins. 
The  equation  describes  the  responses  to  different 
organic  sodium  salts  (see  fig.  11).  From  the  low  values 
of  AF,  the  change  in  free  energy,  the  relative  inde- 
pendence of  response  magnitude  of  temperature  or 
pH,  the  conclusion  is  drawn  that  the  ions  are  loo.seiy 
bound  to  the  taste  receptor  surface  by  a  nonenzymatic 
process  much  like  that  which  occurs  in  the  binding 
of  ions  by  proteins  or  naturally  occurring  polyelec- 
trolytes,  such  as  nucleic  acid  or  polysaccharides  (19). 
Such  binding  may  be  the  initial  step  in  a  series  of 
reactions  leading  ultimately  to  stimulation  of  the 
receptor  and  depolarization  of  the  associated  afferent 
nerve  fiber.  Species  differences  are  attributed  to  dif- 
ferences in  the  detailed  configurations  of  the  reacting 
molecular  sites  on  the  receptor  surfaces. 

SWEET.  The  sweet  taste  appears  to  be  associated 
primarily  with  organic  compounds,  except  for  certain 
inorganic  salts  of  lead  and  beryllium.  The  aliphatic 
hydroxy  compounds  which  include  alcohols,  glycols, 
sugars  and  sugar  derivatives  constitute  one  of  the 
better  known  classes.  Other  stimuli  are  aldehydes, 
ketones,  amides,  esters,  amino  acids,  sulfonic  acids, 
halogenated  hydrocarbons,  etc.  A  sampling  of  thresh- 
old values  for  the  commoner  sweet  stimuli  is  £;iven  in 
table  6. 


The  complex  relations  between  structure  and  the 
.sweet  taste  cannot  be  adequately  explained  by  any 
present  systematization  (sO-  Oertly  &  Myers  C148) 
listed  a  number  of  sweet-producing  molecular  ar- 
rangements and  postulated  that,  to  be  sweet,  a  .sub- 
stance must  contain  a  'glucophore'  and  an  'auxogluc' 
Examples  of  their  analysis  are  given  in  table  7.  How- 
ever, saccharin  and  dulcin  are  but  two  of  the  many 
exceptions. 

In  an  homologous  series,  the  taste  of  the  members 
often  changes  from  sweet  to  bitter  with  increase  in 
molecular  weight.  In  the  higher  members  of  an 
homologous  series,  it  is  said  (144)  that  taste  eventually 
disappears  when  the  products  become  insolufjie.  On 
the  other  hand,  as  previously  noted,  an  increa.se  in 
molecular  weight  associated  with  increasing  chain 
length  in  an  homologous  series  is  paralleled  by  a 
decrease  in  water  solubility  and  an  increase  in  taste 
stimulating  efhciency.  This  was  shown  for  alcohols 
and  glycols  at  threshold  (66).  Some  (62)  have  hy- 
pothesized that  sweetening  power  is  often  a.ssociated 
with  low  water  solubility,  but  numerous  exceptions 
make  this  a  difficult  rule  to  maintain. 

The  importance  of  the  spatial  arrangement  of  the 
molecule  is  strikingly  clear  in  the  case  of  homologues 
in   which   small   changes  may  produce  striking  dif- 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


519 


TABLE  6.  Sweet  Thresholds  in  Man  (in  Molar  Concentrations) 


Substance 

Formula 

Mol.  VVt. 

Median 

Range 

n 

Ref 

Sucrose 

CijHe.Ou 

342.2 

.01  t 

■>7t 

. 005- .016 
.012-. 037 

3 

7 

a,  c,  e 
a,  b,  d 

Glucose 

CcHi.Oe 

180. 1 

.08 

■04-09 

3 

a,  b,  f 

Saccharin  (sodium) 

CO 

/\ 
CeH,    N 

\/ 
SO2 

Na 

+ 

2H2O 

241  .1 

.000023 

. 00002- . 00004 

3 

f.  g 

Beryllium  chloride 

BeClo 

80.0 

.0003 

a,  f 

Sodium  hydroxide 

NaOH 

40. 1 

.008 

.002-. 01 2 

b,  f 

*  a,  Fabian  &  Blum  (74);  b,  Biester  &  Wood  (27);  c,  Schutz  &  Pilgrim  (186),  d,  Janowitz  &  Grossman;  e,  Richter  &  Camp- 
bell (179);  f,  von  Skramlik  (198);  g,  Warren,  R.  W.  &  C.  Pfaffmann  (unpublished  observations). 
t  Detection  threshold. 
J  Recognition  threshold. 


TABLE  7.  Possible  Structural  Basis  for  Sweet  Taste* 


Glucophore 

Auxogluc 

Glycol 

CH.OH— CHOH 

H— 

Glycerol 

CHoOH— CHOH 

CH2OH 

Glucose 

—CO— CHOH 

CHcOH 

Glycine 

COOHCHNH2— 

H— 

Chloroform 

— CCI3 

H— 

Ethyl  nit 

•ate 

— CHONO2 

CH3 

*  According  to 

Oertly  &  Myers  (148). 

ferences  in  ta.ste.  For  example,  of  the  homologues  of 
w-nitroaniline,  which  is  sweet,  only  2-nitro-p-toluidine 
is  sweet. 


Sweet 
NO2 


Sweet 
NO2 


Tasteless 


NH, 


NH., 


NO, 


NH., 


CH, 


CH3 


Very  shghtly  bitter 
NH, 


NO, 


CH, 


Saccharin   is  one  of  the  better  known   physiologi- 
cally inert  synthetic  sweetening  agents.  The  salts  of 


saccharin,  especially  the  sodium  salt  crystallose,  are 
sweet  presumably  due  to  the  anion 


■N- 


-SO, 
-CO 


Where  substitution  of  the  hydrogen  in  the  imide 
group  occurs  to  form  .^''-methyl  saccharin,  the  com- 
pound is  tasteless  presumably  because  ionization 
cannot  occur, 


CO. 


-SO, 


.NCH,. 


Several  other  intensely  sweet  substances  are  dulcin, 
cyclamate  (Sucaryl)  and  the  4-alkoxy-3-amino- 
nitrobenzenes.  The  n  propyl  derivative  of  the  latter 
class,  called  P-4000,  is  said  to  be  the  sweetest  known 
compound  but  its  use  as  a  synthetic  sweetening  agent 
is  limited  by  its  toxicity  (29,  131). 

Stereoisomerism  is  of  significance  in  taste  as  in 
other  physiological  systems.  In  fact,  one  of  the  earlier 
examples  of  the  biological  significance  of  optical 
activity  was  provided  by  asparagine  of  which  the 
dextro  forin  is  sweet,  the  levo  form  tasteless.  Freshly 
prepared  solutions  of  alpha  D  glucose  are  sweeter 
than  beta  D  glucose  which  predominates  in  solution 
after  mutarotation  has  occurred  (44). 

Mention  has  already  been  made  of  the  selective 
and  reversible  action  of  certain  drugs  like  gymnemic 
acid  which  reduces  sensitivity  to  sweet  and  bitter  but 


520 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


leaves  salt  and  sour  unaffected  (191).  The  effect  may 
be  demonstrated  most  simply  after  chewing  a  few  of 
the  dried  leaves.  A  similar  action  is  described  for 
verba  santa,  an  extract  of  the  leaves  of  Eriodicyton 
calif ornicum.  Topical  application  of  w^eak  concentra- 
tions of  cocaine  depress  taste  in  the  order,  bitter  > 
sweet  >  salt  >  sour.  Stovaine  in  proper  concentra- 
tions will  eliininate  sweet  and  Ijitter  and  reduce 
sensitivity  to  salt  and  acid  followed  by  hypergeusia 
for  salt.  Cocaine  ageusia  may  be  followed  ijy  hyper- 
geusia for  sweet  and  bitter.  The  Sudanese  plant 
Bumelia  duliifica  is  said  to  change  sweet  and  bitter  to 
sour. 

The  extract  from  the  Gvnimmn  leaves,  after  purifi- 
cation and  recrystallization,  yields  a  white  powder 
with  a  melting  point  of  i99°C  and  a  molecular  weight 
of  805.  This  appears  to  be  a  glycoside  which  yields 
glucose,  arabinose  and  a  small  quantity  of  glucuro- 
nalactone  upon  hydrolysis.  The  hydrolysate  has  no 
effect  on  taste.  Other  sul^stances  in  the  crude  extract 
do  not  influence  the  inhiljitory  action.  Not  only  is 
the  sweet  taste  of  such  diverse  substances  as  sucrose 
and  saccharin  suppressed,  both  are  equalh'  reduced 
in  sweetness,  i.e.  at  suprathreshold  levels  equal  sweet- 
ness matches  remain  unchanged  (Warren,  R.  M.  & 
C.   Pfaffmann,  unpublished  observations). 

The  differential  action  of  this  drug  is  clearly  shown 
in  the  electrical  activity  of  the  chorda  tympani  nerve 
(fig.  12),  although  the  effect  does  not  last  as  long  as 
the  perceptual  effect  in  man.  A  similar  reversible  ef- 
fect can  be  produced  by  the  salts  of  heavy  metals, 
silver  and  mercury,  but  not  by  arsenic,  arsenous  acids 
or  potassium  ferricyanide.  This  suggests  competitive 
blocking  of  a  mechanism  which  is  nonenzymatic  (88). 
These  results  are  in  striking  agreement  with  those 
obtained  in  invertebrate  contact  chemoception  (67, 
68).  The  evidence  from  functional  studies  contra- 
indicates  an  enzymatic  process  in  stimulation  gener- 


!i!SO 


>eo. 


3 


r-^ 


10  MIN. 


i-i     .1   No  CI 

•—»    .1  sue. 


T r 

TIME  (mir  ) 


1 r 

l-IO  min    H 


FIG.  12.  Differential  suppression  of  taste  response  to  sucrose 
(«/<r.)  and  sodium  chloride  (chorda  tympani  discharge)  by 
gymnemic  acid  after  10  min.  application  to  surface  of  tongue. 
[From  Hagstrom  (88).] 


ally  in  spite  of  the  demonstration  of  enzymes  in  the 
neighborhood  of  taste  cells  (14). 

Action  potential  studies  have  shown  that  an  indi- 
\idual  afferent  receptor  neural  unit  might  respond 
not  only  to  sugar  but  also  to  a  salt  like  .sodium  chlo- 
ride. Thus,  specificity  to  a  chemical  agent  must  be 
specificity  of  sites  within  or  on  the  individual  sense 
cells.  Presumably,  different  sites  are  specifically 
sensiti\e  to  sugar  on  the  one  hand  and  salt  on  the 
other  (in  addition  to  a  wide  variety  of  other  sub- 
stances). Gymnemic  acid  does  not  block  the  receptor 
cell  as  a  whole  but  onl\'  the  sucrose  site. 

Threshold  and  suprathreshold  ecjual-sweetne.ss  com- 
parison methods  have  been  employed  to  study  rela- 
tive sweetness  of  different  stimuli.  Although  the 
exact  values  may  vary  from  experimenter  to  experi- 
menter, the  relative  order  of  sweetness  among  the 
sugars,  for  example,  is  the  same.  In  equimolar  con- 
centrations the  order  of  sweetness  is:  sucro.se  >  fruc- 
tose >  maltose  >  glucose  >  lactose  (44).  The  rela- 
tion between  concentration  and  sweetness  changes 
with  concentration  (see  fig.  13).  There  are  very  wide 
differences  in  the  stimulating  efficiency  of  sweet 
stimuli.  Close  to  threshold,  saccharin  is  500  to  700 
times  less  concentrated  than  sucrose  of  equal  sweet- 
ness. As  yet  there  is  no  indication  why  .some  syn- 
thetic agents  are  so  effective.  In  general,  the  available 
evidence  suggests  that  the  initial  step  in  sweet  stimu- 
lation ma\'  be  a  process  like  that  already  elaborated 
for  salts.  The  response  to  sugar  is  resistant  to  enzyme 
poisons  and  pH  change  but  not  to  surface  active 
competiti\e  inhibitors. 

BITTER.  Bitter,  like  sweet,  is  elicited  by  members  of 
many  chemical  classes  and  is  often  found  in  associa- 
tion with  sweet  and  other  taste  qualities.  Increasing 
molecular  weight  of  inorganic  salts  is  associated  with 
increasing  bitterness  (see  p.  516).  An  increase  in 
length  of  the  carbon  chain  of  the  organic  molecules 
may  he  associated  with  a  change  from  sweet  to  Ijitter. 
Many  sweet  substances  have  a  concomitant  bitter 
taste  or  aftertaste  (e.g.  saccharin).  This  douiile  or 
multiple  taste  quality  is  especially  apparent  as  the 
stimulus  moves  from  the  front  to  the  back  of  the 
tongue  where  bitter  sensitivity  is  especially  developed. 
The  best  known  class  of  bitter  substances  is  the 
alkaloids  which  are  complex  nitrogenous  compounds, 
often  highly  toxic,  such  as  quinine,  caffeine,  strych- 
nine and  nicotine  (144).  Nitro  compounds  are  often 
bitter  (such  as  picric  acid)  especially  if  three  or  some- 
times two  nitro  groups  are  present.  The  following 
groups  are  often  associated  with  bitter  taste :  (NO2)  > 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE  ^2 1 


-  0.075 


E  0.050 


S  0.025 


0.050  0  075  0  100 

Gram-mol  per  cent 


0125 


0150 


FIG.  13.  Curves  showing  the  concentrations  at  which  different  substances  taste  as  sweet  as  various 
concentrations  of  sucrose.  Gram-mol  per  cent  is  '  fo  the  value  of  the  molar  concentration.  [From 
Cameron  C44).j 


TABLE  8.  Bitter  Thresholds  in  Man  (in  Molar  Concentrations) 

Substance 
Quinine  sulphate 
Quin  ine  hydrochloride 
Strychine  inonohydrochloride 
Nicotine 
Caffeine 
Phenyl  thiourea  (PTC) 

tasters 

nontastcrs 
Urea 
Magnesium  sulfate 

(Epsom  salt) 

*  a,  Blakeslee  (28);  b,  Hanig  (96);  c,  Harris  (99);  d,  Hartmann  (of.  48);  e,  Harris  &  Kalmus  (100);  f,  Kiesow  (118);  g,  Parker 
&  Stabler  (154);  h,  Richter  &  Clisby  (180);  i,  Schutz  &  Pilgrim  (186);  j,  Setterfield,  Schottl  &  Snyder  (cf.  48):  k,  von  Skram- 
lik  (198). 

t  Modes. 


Formula 

Mol.  \Vt. 

Median 

Range 

n 

Ref 

(C2oH,,N.002H.,S04 

746.90 

. 000008 

.0000004-. 0000 ! I 

3 

b, 

f,  k 

CaciHo4N.,0,HCl 

360.88 

.00003 

.  000002- .  0004 

3 

g. 

k 

C2,H.,.,N.>0;HCI 

370-75 

, 00000 1 6 

I 

k 

C5H4NC4H7NCH:, 

1 62  . 2 

.0000 1 9 

I 

k 

CsHioN^Ot! 

.94.1 

.0007 

.0003-. 001 

2 

i, 

k 

CeHsNHCSNH. 

I  52 . 2  I 

. 00002 t\ 

.oo8t     / 

f. 0000002  to 
I  >-oi7 

6 

a. 

c,  d, 

CO  (NHO2 

60.  I 

.  12 

. 116-.13 

2 

c, 

k 

MgSOi  7H2O 

246.49 

.0046 

.0042-. 005 

2 

k 

2,  =N,  =N=  — SH,  — S— ,  — S— S— ,  and  — CS— . 
Some  typical  threshold  values  for  the  human  are 
shown  in  table  8. 

The  importance  of  structure  and  specific  chemical 
grouping  is  shown  by  the  phenomenon  of 'taste  blind- 
ness,' a  specific  relative   insensitivity  to  a  number  of 

S 


substances  possessing  the   y  NC —  group.  Phenylthio- 

carbamide  (PTC)  is  widely  used  as  a  test  for  'taste 
blindness.'  The  actual  distribution  of  taste-blind 
individuals  found  depends  upon  the  manner  of  ad- 
ministering the  PTC;,  the  percentages  of  'nontasters' 
in  Caucasians  having  been  variously  reported  to  be 
between  3  per  cent  and  40  per  cent.  A  graded  series 


of  solutions  yields  no  sharp  cutoff  at  one  threshold 
concentration  but  rather  a  bimodal  distribution. 

This  deficiency  is  inherited  as  a  Mendelian  reces- 
sive characteristic.  Evidence  for  such  taste  defects 
were  found  in  anthropoid  apes  (28,  48,  too).  Rats, 
however,  rejected  PTC  solutions  with  no  evidence  of 
the  defect.  PTC  is  intensely  toxic  to  the  rat  when 
administered  by  stomach  tube.  This  property  was 
utilized  in  the  preparation  of  a  rat  poison  "Antu' 
from  a  chemically  related  but  tasteless  compound 
C178,  180). 

S 

\      " 
The  inability  to  taste   y  NC —  may  be  overridden 

by  other  chemical  groups  as  in  thiourea,  NH2CSNH.2, 


522 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOCi'    I 


which  is  sour  to  ail  persons.  Many  of  the  substances 
with  whicli  taste  blindness  may  be  demonstrated  are 
antithyroid  compoimds  (99).  Taste  blindness  is  not 
correlated  with  sensitivity  for  other  bitter  stimuli  or 
the  other  taste  qualities.  This  suggests  a  high  degree 
of  specificity  for  the  particular  chemical  linkage  to- 
gether with  some  feature  of  the  receptor  mechanism. 
An  attempt  has  been  made  to  relate  taste  blindness 
to  solubility  of  PTC  in  saliva  (47).  In  view  of  the 
specificity  of  the  linkage  this  does  not  seem  to  be  a 
likely  explanation. 

The  fact  that  sweet  and  bitter  sensitivity  are  often 
associated  in  the  case  of  certain  stimuli  and  that 
both  tend  to  be  inactivated  by  the  action  of  drugs  or 
narcotic  agents  led  one  in\estigator  to  propose  that 
both  depend  upon  the  action  of  a  single  receptor 
mechanism  (194).  Of  all  the  taste  mechanisms,  that 
underlying  bitter  sensitivity  is  least  well  understood. 

ELECTRIC  TASTE.  That  electric  currents  can  stimulate 
the  sense  of  taste  has  been  known  almost  since  the 
discovery  of  electricity.  \'olta  noted,  for  example,  a 
sour  taste  when  a  circuit  with  two  dissimilar  metals 
made  contact  with  the  tongue.  Such  taste  is  elicited 
not  only  at  the  make  and  ijreak  of  current  but  by 
the  steady  flow  as  contrasted  with  the  more  familiar 
stimulation  of  nervous  tissue  by  short  duration  pulses. 
Anodal  unipolar  stimulation  of  the  tongue  with  an 
indifTerent  electrode  elsewhere  on  the  body  elicits  a 
sour  taste;  cathodal  stimulation  yields  a  complex 
alkaline  quality  but,  at  cathodal  break,  sour  is  re- 
ported (198). 

Early  investigators  often  employed  metallic  polariz- 
able  electrodes.  With  an  inert  electrode  like  platinum, 
the  following  electrolytic  change  occurs  when  current 
flows  through  a  weak  salt  solution.  Electrons  are 
introduced  at  the  cathode  toward  which  the  positive 
hydrogen  and  sodium  ions  are  attracted.  A  higher 
voltage  is  usually  required  to  discharge  the  Na  ion 
so  that  H  ions  are  discharged  and  H2  is  liberated, 

ae  +  2H2O  -^  2OH-  +  H.. 

leaving  Na+  and  OH"  ions.  At  the  positive  pole 
electrons  come  primarily  from  the  OH~  ions  which 
by  their  discharge  leave  an  excess  of  H+  ions. 


4OH- 


2H,0  +  O2  +  2e 


These  together  with  the  remaining  CI"  ions  form  a 
dilute  solution  of  hydrochloric  acid.  These  chemical 
effects  would  appear  to  account  for  the  sour  taste  at 


the  anode  and  alkaline  taste  at  the  cathode.  In  addi- 
tion there  is  movement  of  cations  toward  the  cathode 
and  anions  toward  the  anode  with  a  resulting  change 
in  concentration  in  the  vicinity  of  the  electrodes. 

With  a  nonpolarizable  reversible  electrode,  electron 
transfer  at  the  electrode  is  derived  from  the  reaction 
Ag  ^  Ag+  -f  e.  No  discharge  of  OH"  or  H+  ions 
occurs.  There  is  no  electrolysis,  l)ut  the  subject  still 
reports  sour  at  the  anode  (40).  Further  work  is  de- 
sirable in  view  of  one  preliminar)-  report  that  the 
anode  produces  a  salty  taste  when  a  carefully  con- 
structed reversible  electrode  is  utilized  C71). 

In  general  two  hypotheses  have  been  proposed  to 
account  for  the  electric  taste.  The  first  is  the  chemical 
theory  in  which  it  is  belie\ed  that  taste  buds  are 
stimulated  by  the  concentration  of  ions  resulting  from 
electrolysis.  Thus  the  sour  of  the  platinum  electrode 
is  said  to  be  due  to  the  excess  of  hydrogen  ions.  The 
appearance  of  the  same  taste  with  a  reversible  elec- 
trode suggests  the  second  \iew,  namely  that  direct 
depolarization  of  the  taste  membrane  occurs  by 
virtue  of  the  ionic  transfer  in  the  cell  and  across  the 
cell  membrane.  In  both  cases,  of  course,  the  pa.ssage 
of  current  is  an  electrochemical  reaction. 

Direct  stimulation  of  the  nerve  fibers  so  that  the 
receptor  organ  itself  is  'by-passed'  can  be  ruled  out, 
at  least  for  direct  current  anodal  currents.  The  taste 
threshold  current  is  lower  for  the  anode  than  the 
cathode,  which  is  the  reverse  of  the  relation  found 
for  direct  nerve  stimulation.  Furthermore  the  eleva- 
tion of  threshold  after  the  topical  applications  of 
tetracaine  was  much  greater  for  the  anode  than  the 
cathode,  suggesting  that  the  anode  stimulated  the 
more  superficial  receptor  but  that  the  cathode  stimu 
la  ted  the  deeper  nerve  fibers.  The  strength  duration 
curses  of  taste  indicate  a  longer  time  constant  for 
the  anode  than  the  cathode  (38,  40). 

Electrophysiological  recordings  of  the  single  taste 
fibers  show  that  anodal  polarization  of  the  tongue 
surface  causes  a  discharge  like  that  to  chemical  stimu- 
lation, except  that  the  latency  to  the  electrical  stimulus 
is  approximately  5  to  7  msec,  whereas  that  to  chemi- 
cal stimulation  is  approximately  35  to  50  msec.  (160). 
Thus  the  anodal  electrical  stimulus  appears  to  act 
via  the  receptor  cell  but  with  a  much  shorter  latency 
as  though  some  initial  step  were  by-passed. 

The  discharge  to  a  steady  anodal  current  continues, 
after  an  initial  decrement,  as  long  as  the  current  flows. 
The  same  magnitude  of  cathodal  current,  however, 
causes  an  immediate  inhibition  of  activity  which  lasts 
as  long  as  the  current  flows.  Upon  break  of  the 
cathodal  current,   there  follows  a  transient  burst  of 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE  523 


activity.  Other  sense  organs  appear  to  show  the  same 
polarity  relations,  e.g.  the  tactile  receptors  of  the 
frog  skin  and  photo  receptors  of  the  Limulus  eye, 
suggesting  that  these  effects  are  not  specific  to  taste 
(138,  139).  These  effects  are  just  opposite  to  those 
seen  in  the  axon  where  anodal  block  and  cathodal 
excitation  are  found. 

Studies  of  alternating  current  stimulation  support 
the  view  that  the  receptor  ceil  mediates  the  electric 
taste  at  low  frequencies  from  30  to  50  cps  at  which 
sour  predominates.  The  low  frequencies  presumably 
can  stimulate  the  receptor  organs  with  their  slower 
time  constants.  High  frequencies  around  1000  cps 
elicit  a  more  complex  bitter  taste.  The  high  fre- 
quencies may  stimulate  ti.ssues  with  faster  time  con- 
stants, i.e.  the  nerve  fibers  themselves  (41). 

Intermittent  square  wave  stimulation  has  been 
employed  in  studies  of  the  so-called  'flicker  fusion'  of 
taste.  The  original  studies  (6)  purporting  to  demon- 
strate gustatory  flicker  fusion  at  frequencies  between 
125  and  350  cps  have  not  been  confirmed  Cii3>  i  70j 
183),  although  it  is  true  that  something  akin  to  fusion 
can  be  reported.  The  effect  appears  to  be  largely  a 
tactual  phenomenon  which  can  be  demonstrated  in 
regions  of  the  mouth  and  lips  where  no  taste  buds  are 
found  (170).  Reports  of  true  taste  fusion  (38)  occur 
with  extremely  low  frequencies  in  the  range  of  from 
0.3  to  10  cps. 

Parameters  of  Slimiilaliori 

TEMPERATURE.  The  rate  of  most  chemical  reactions 
is  increased  by  a  rise  in  temperature.  Early  work 
showed  that  taste  was  optimal  in  a  middle  range 
variously  reported  between  10°  and  20°C,  20°  and 
30°C  and  30°  and  40 °C.  At  the  extremes  of  0°  or 
50 °C  the  tongue  is  nearly  insensitive  especially  after 
it  has  been  immersed  in  solution  (198).  Komuro 
(125)  studied  intermediate  values  and  reported  a 
drop  in  threshold  with  temperature  rise  from  10°  to 
30°C,  with  some  suggestion  of  an  increase  beyond 
30 °C  for  all  stimuli.  Chinaglia  (46)  found  no  change 
in  threshold  but  did  find  a  change  in  reaction  time. 
The  interpretation  of  reaction  time  data  is  somewhat 
equivocal  because,  in  another  study  (143),  no  corre- 
lation was  found  between  reaction  time  and  thresh- 
old for  sodium  chloride  over  a  wide  temperature 
range. 

Goudrian  (86)  showed  that  the  apparent  taste 
intensity  of  sugar  solutions  increased  with  increasing 
temperature  between  10°  and  40°C.  Acid  showed  a 


FIG.  14.  The  effect  of  temperature  on  taste  thresholds  for 
sodium  chloride,  quinine  sulphate,  dulcin  and  hydrochloric  acid. 
The  ordinate  gives  the  thresholds  in  arbitrary  units.  The  value 
of  one  unit  on  the  ordinate  differs  for  each  of  the  four  sub- 
stances, as  shown  by  the  key  in  the  figure.  For  example,  one 
unit  for  NaCI  equals  0.0005  P^''  cent.  [From  Hahn  (93).] 


similar  but  not  as  regular  or  as  striking  a  change. 
Salt  and  quinine  fell  in  intensity  with  an  optimum  for 
salt  at  io°C.  The  optimum  for  quinine  was  less  con- 
sistent. 

One  of  the  better  controlled  studies  is  that  of  Hahn 
&  Gunther  (gi)  on  absolute  thresholds  at  different 
temperatures  using  the  'Geschmackslupe.'  This  device 
restricts  the  flow  of  solution  to  a  specified  region  of 
the  tongue  so  that  a  preadapting  flow  of  water  at  the 
same  temperature  can  be  employed  (see  fig.  14). 
Sugar  sensitivity  increases,  salt  and  quinine  decrease 
and  acid  is  unaffected  by  temperature  rise.  The 
greatest  deviations  occurred  with  different  sweet 
stimuli.  Glycol  showed  little  change  with  tempera- 
ture whereas  beryllium  salts  and  other  sugars  followed 
the  sucrose  curve.  Certain  acids  showed  slight  up- 
turn at  either  temperature  extreme  and  different 
salts  showed  a  flattening  out  or  even  a  fall  beyond 
37°C.  With  different  bitter  stimuli  there  was  only  an 
increase  in  slope.  Hahn  &  Gunther's  values  do  not 
include  the  extremes  which  cause  insensitivity.  If 
extended,  their  curves  would  probably  have  shown 
a  rise  in  threshold  at  the  low  and  high  values. 


524 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


One  study  among  recent  ones  on  the  temperature 
effect  using  an  electrophysiological  method  reported 
no  change  in  response  magnitude  for  sodium  chloride 
at  temperatures  of  20°,  25°  and  30°C  (19)-  Another 
study  with  the  larger  temperature  range  found  only 
a  10  per  cent  variation  in  magnitude  response  between 
20°  and  30°C,  but  larger  effects  outside  these  limits 
with  sodium  chloride  (i).  There  was  a  sharp  rise 
from  15°  to  an  optimum  of  22 °C  with  a  gradual 
fall  from  22°C  tb  37°C  and  a  greater  drop  at  45 °C. 
Since  a  fall  in  neural  response  magnitude  is  equiva- 
lent to  a  rise  in  threshold,  the  electrophysiological 
results  are  in  agreement  with  the  human  data  showing 
that  sodium  chloride  sensitivity  falls  off  with  tem- 
perature rise,  particularly  above  22°C. 

In  interpreting  the  temperature  effects  it  should  be 
remembered  that  biological  systems  have  a  normal 
functional  range  beyond  which  biological  arrest  usu- 
ally occurs.  With  extreme  cooling  or  excessive  heating 
there  may  be  irreversible  changes  (22).  Thus  bio- 
logical systems  with  a  mid-temperature  region  of 
optimal  function,  in  general,  yield  U-shaped  functions. 
It  was  the  optimal  range  with  which  earlier  workers 
were  concerned.  The  more  careful  study  of  systematic 
changes  within  normal  limits  shows  clearly  that 
temperature  increase  does  not  increase  all  taste 
sensitivity.  There  is  no  simple  temperature  coefficient 
in  the  usual  sense. 

AREA  AND  DURATION.  Stimulation  of  single  papillae 
or  of  a  limited  area  of  the  tongue  by  a  single  drop  of 
solution  usually  results  in  higher  thresholds  or  less 
intense  suprathreshold  tastes  than  does  tasting  by  the 
whole  mouth  (44,  198).  The  expression  IS'^  =  K 
approximately  describes  the  relation  between  thresh- 
old intensity,  /,  and  surface  area,  S,  with  exponents 
of  0.73  for  sodium  chloride,  0.6  for  citric  acid,  0.93 
for  sucrose,  and  i  .42  for  quinine  hydrochloride. 
Threshold  decreases  with  areas  up  to  60  to  90  mm-. 
A  similar  relation  also  holds  for  the  apparent  in- 
tensity of  suprathreshold  solutions  (43)- 

The  relation  between  threshold  and  stimulus  dura- 
tion can  be  expressed  as  t  =  C/i"  where  /  is  duration, 
i  is  threshold  and  C  is  a  constant.  For  sodium  chloride 
and  citric  acid,  n  equals  1.5,  for  sucrose  n  equals  2.0. 
A  similar  relation  with  shorter  durations  holds  for 
the  electric  taste  (33).  Similarly  the  apparent  in- 
tensity of  suprathreshold  solutions  depends  upon 
duration.  With  long  durations,  the  sensation  of  taste 
waxes  slowly,  reaching  a  maximum  for  quinine  in  '8 
to  10  sec.  and  for  salt  in  4  to  5  sec.  With  electric 
taste  the  'build-up'  time  is  i  to  1.5  sec.  C42). 


REACTION  TIME.  Most  early  workers  report  that  bitter 
yields  the  longest,  and  salty  the  shortest  times  with 
sugar  and  acid  intermediate  (198).  Because  the 
stimulus  intensity  influences  reaction  time  (169),  it  is 
necessary  to  specify  this  parameter.  One  recent  study 
(32,  34)  utilized  a  flow  system  in  conjunction  with 
an  electrical  measurement  of  the  solution  flow  at  the 
tongue  surface  or  onset  of  current,  in  the  case  of  the 
electric  taste.  Reaction  times  vary  with  different 
qualities  and  within  the  same  quality.  Reaction  time 
is  longest  at  threshold  and  shortest  at  the  higher  in- 
tensities, often  by  a  factor  of  three  or  more.  It  seems 
reasonable,  in  \  icw  of  the  uneven  distribution  of 
sensitivity  over  the  tongue  surface,  that  reaction  time 
for  different  stimulus  classes  would  vary  with  the 
region  stimulated.  This  parameter  has  not  been  in- 
vestigated, however.  An  increase  both  in  the  area  of 
stimulated  surface  or  of  hydrostatic  pressure  of  the 
solution  against  the  tongue  surface  decrea.ses  reaction 
time   to   some   extent  (108). 

Adaptation 

The  continued  flow  of  taste  solution  over  the  tongue 
leads  to  a  diminution  in  subjective  intensity  and  an 
elevation  of  the  absolute  threshold  which  is  propor- 
tional to  the  intensity  of  the  adapting  stimulus  (see 
fig.  15).  The  rate  and  form  of  the  adaptation  curve 
within    the   same    quality    may   vary   with    different 


FIG.  15.  Adaptation  and  recovery  curves  for  NaCl.  The 
ordinate  indicates  the  threshold  concentrations.  The  course  of 
adaptation  to  three  concentrations  of  NaCl,  5,  10  and  15  per 
cent,  is  shown  for  an  adaptation  period  of  30  sec.  and  a  re- 
covery period  of  30  sec.  The  unadapted  threshold  is  0.24  per 
cent.  [From  Hahn  (92). j 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


0^0 


Stimuli,  l)ut  the  recovery  curves  tend  to  have  the 
same  shape  (92).  Subthreshold  stimuli  may  elevate 
the  threshold  in  a  similar  manner  but  to  a  lesser  de- 
cree. Adaptation  by  acid  adapts  the  sour  taste  for 
other  acids,  but  in  the  case  of  bitter  and  sweet  stimuli 
cross  adaptation  within  these  respective  qualities  may- 
occur  only  between  some  but  not  all  stimuli.  More 
striking,  however,  is  the  case  of  salt  where  no  cross 
adaptation  was  found  among  24  inorganic  salts 
studied  (94). 

It  has  commonly  been  assumed  that  sensory  adapta- 
tion reflects  the  exhaustion  of  some  receptive  sub- 
stance in  the  cell  in  a  manner  analogous  to  the 
bleaching  of  visual  purple  by  light.  The  combination 
of  the  stimulus  with  such  a  receptive  substance  was 
assumed  to  be  necessary  for  stimulation  (129),  and 
further  that  all  stimuli  eliciting  the  same  quality 
would  be  mediated  by  the  same  receptor  substance 
so  that  cross  adaptation  would  result.  Such  a  mecha- 
nism for  the  salts  would  require  24  different  receptive 
substances  in  the  taste  cell.  Hahn  rejected  this  notion 
and  hypothesized  a  specific  inhibition  of  the  cell  re- 
ceptor membrane  (change  in  permeability)  for  the 
adapting  stimulus  only.  The  receptor  cell  itself  was 
not  rendered  inexcitable  (94).  Analogous  results  were 
found  in  recent  electrophysiological  studies  (20).  No 
cross  adaptation  between  calciuin  chloride  and  so- 
dium chloride  for  example,  was  found,  even  though 
the  single  fiber  analysis  shows  that  calcium  chloride 
and  sodium  chloride  affected  the  same  peripheral 
fiber  and  receptor  cell  (75). 

Bujas  has  pointed  out  that  the  subjective  intensity 
of  taste  does  not  always  parallel  the  peripheral  process. 
Maximal  subjective  intensity  develops  only  after  the 
stimulus  has  been  acting  for  some  seconds.  During 
this  'buildup'  period,  however,  the  receptor  sensi- 
tivity is  falling  as  shown  by  the  rise  in  threshold. 
Subjective  intensity  begins  to  fall  off^  only  later, 
showing  that  the  magnitude  of  sensation  is  probably 
the  result  of  a  central  and  peripheral  process  working 
in  opposition  (37).  Beidler  has  noted  that  the  main- 
tained steady  discharge  for  sodium  chloride  in  the 
electrophysiological  record  is  at  variance  with  the 
complete  disappearance  of  salt  sensation  reported 
for  all  but  the  strongest  concentrations  in  the  human 
observer  (2,  39,  127).  This  points  to  a  process  of 
central  adaptation. 

Adaptation  to  sucrose  or  sodium  chloride  enhances 
sensitivity  to  stimuli  eliciting  other  qualities.  Adapta- 
tion by  quinine  enhances  sensitivity  to  sour  and  salt, 
but  adaptation  by  hydrochloric  acid  does  not  affect 
the  other  qualities  (64,    140).   It  is  well  known  that 


distilled  water  appears  sweet  following  a  weak  acid. 
The  recent  finding  that  water  acts  as  a  stimulus  for 
certain  taste  endings  and  that  the  magnitude  of  such 
discharge  can  be  modified  by  prior  treatment  with 
acids  or  other  chemical  stimuli  suggests  a  peripheral 
locus  for  some  of  these  effects  (136,  164). 

A  positive  after  taste,  i.e.  persistence  of  the  same 
taste  quality  after  withdrawal  of  the  stimulus,  has 
been  attributed  usually  to  residual  taste  particles  in 
the  mouth  or  to  slow  dcsorption  from  the  receptor 
surface. 

Interaction  when  two  disparate  areas  of  the  tongue 
are  stimulated  has  been  reported.  Weak  acid  or  sugar 
solutions  were  said  to  reduce  the  threshold  for  salt 
on  the  opposite  side  (119).  An  enhancement  of  salt 
sensitivity  occurs  with  weak  sugar  solutions  but 
inhibition  or  elevation  of  salt  threshold  occurs  with 
stronger  sugar  concentrations.  Such  effects  with 
stimulation  of  disparate  sensory  surfaces  must  have  a 
central  origin.  Successive  contrast  effects  of  a  similar 
nature  also  have  been  described  (36,  198). 

Unfortunately,  there  have  been  few  systematic 
studies  of  masking  and  interactions  with  taste  mix- 
tures, except  for  efforts  to  duplicate  complex  tastes 
by  mixing  four  components.  One  well-known  inter- 
action is  the  reduction  of  sourness  by  the  addition  of 
sugar  or  other  sweetener.  This  has  been  studied  by 
the  electrophysiological  method.  The  discharge  of  a 
nerve  strand  to  a  mixture  of  10  per  cent  sucrose  and 
an  acid  of  pH  2.5  showed  only  an  increase  compared 
to  the  response  to  the  sugar  or  acid  individually  (10). 
There  was  no  peripheral  inhibition.  Such  sour-sweet 
interaction,  therefore,  must  have  a  central  locus. 

Intensitive  Relations 

Differential  sensitivity  (A/  /)  as  found  by  different 
investigators  is  summarized  in  table  9.  Values  from 
i/io  (10  per  cent)  to  i  i  (100  per  cent)  with  a 
modal  value  of  i  '5  (20  per  cent)  have  been  reported 
depending  upon  the  intensity  level,  amount  of  stimu- 
lus, criterion  of  judgment,  etc.,  employed.  Constancy 
of  A///  with  intensity  has  been  reported  by  some, 
whereas  others  reported  a  decrease  in  differential 
sensitivity  at  the  high  or  low  intensities,  but  the 
change  in  these  latter  instances  was  relatively  small 
(approximately  10  times)  compared  with  the  too  to 
1000  times  change  found  for  vision  and  hearing.  High 
differential  sensitivity  for  one  taste  quality  is  not 
correlated  with  high  sensitivity  for  others,  and  dif- 
ferences between  subjects  may  be  greater  than  the 


526 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOG\'    I 


TABLE  9.  Differential  Thresholds  {Al/I) 
in  Taste  Modalities 


No.  of 
Subjects 

Sweet 
(Sucrose) 

Salt 
(NaCl) 

Bitter 

(CaSeine) 

Sour  (Cit- 
ric Acid) 

• 

OS 

2 
2 

6-8 
6 
2 

10 

9' 
3.1 

5'   (e.gj 

4-8'V3.7/ 

I 
5-8 

I 

6.6 

I 
3.8 

6.7 
I 

7.15 

3 

I 

4-5 
I 

6.7 

4-7 

I 
3-3 

I 
5-2 

I 
4-5 

e 
b 

f 
c 

g 

§ 
d 

a 

h 

Range  in 
individ- 
ual sub- 
jects 

I      I 
8     2 

I         I 
10     2.5 

I          I 
6.7     1. 15 

I  I 

II  1.6 

h 

Median 
fraction 

Median 
ratio 

I 
5 

20% 

I 
6.6 

15% 

I 
4 

25% 

I 
4.8 

21% 

*  a,  Beidler  (20);  b,  Bujas  (35);  c,  Fodor  &  Happisch 
(76);  d,  Holway  &  Hurvich  (107);  e,  Kopera  (126);  f,  Lem- 
berger  (133);  g,  SanduUah  (185);  h,  Schutz  &  Pilgrim  (186). 

t  Saccharin.         |  Quinine. 

§  Krogh  &  Jensen  cited  in  (126).  Values  obtained  by 
Keppler  (117)  were  much  lower  than  most  values  obtained 
by  above  authors  and  have  been  omitted  from  the   table. 


differences  in  the  same  subject  for  different  intensities 
or  qualities  (186). 

Attempts  to  measure  or  scale  subjective  taste  in- 
tensity have  employed  different  methods.  One  is  the 
summation  of  just  noticeable  difference  steps  (JND's). 
In  one  study,  successive  JND  steps  were  determined 
for  two  sweet  substances,  crystallose  (sodium  sac- 
charin) and  sucrose  so  that  stimuli  falling  at  equal 
JND  steps  abo\e  threshold  could  be  specified  (133). 
These  concentrations,  however,  were  not  equallv 
sweet  when  directly  compared.  At  high  concentra- 
tions, saccharin  became  relatively  less  sweet  than 
sucrose  of  an  equal  JND  scale  \alue.  As  in  the  case 
of  other  modalities,  the  JND  summation  scale  is  not 


a  valid  scale  for  subjecti\e  magnitude  (193).  This  is 
also  true  when  cross  quality  comparisons,  e.g.  salti- 
ness versus  sweetness,  are  carried  out  (35). 

Another  method,  analogous  to  the  equal  loudness 
measurement  in  hearing,  utilizes  the  direct  match 
between  one  solution  and  an  arbitrarily  selected 
set  of  standards  (44,  158).  For  sweetness,  a  series  of 
sucrose  solutions  is  often  used;  for  sourness,  hydro- 
chloric acid,  etc.  This  does  not  give  taste  intensity 
directly,  onK  the  relati\e  taste  effectiveness  of  dif- 
ferent substances  in  eliciting  equal  taste  intensities. 

In  recent  years,  a  number  of  direct  scaling  methods 
have  been  developed,  stemming  from  the  work  in 
audition.  In  one  series  of  studies  (17,  134)  the  frac- 
tionation method  showed  that  subjectise  magnitude 
increased  directK  with  the  physical  concentration,  a 
special  case  of  Stevens'  general  psychophysical  law, 
^  =  s",  where  ^  is  sensation,  s  is  stimulus  intensity, 
and  n  is  an  exponent,  the  exponent  in  this  case  being 
equal  to  one  (193).  Other  studies  using  similar 
methods  found  that  taste  intensity  increased  as  an 
exponential  function  of  stimulus  concentration,  so 
that  the  exact  relation  between  taste  intensity  and 
stimulus  concentration  is  yet  to  be  established  (187). 

Glucose  is  less  sweet,  molecule  for  molecule,  than 
sucrose.  Furthermore  the  ratios  of  concentrations  for 
equal  sweetness  of  the  two  sugars  change  with  concen- 
tration. Sweetness  does  not  increase  equally  with 
concentration  for  both  (137).  That  this  depends  upon 
some  basic  receptor  mechanism  is  suggested  by  the 
fact  that  the  electrophysiological  response  in  the 
chorda  t\nipani  nerve  of  at  least  one  species,  the  rat, 
follows  a  rather  different  course  for  glucose  than  for 
sucrose  as  shown  in  figure  16  (89). 

These  relations  also  bear  upon  another  effect,  the 
so-called  'supplemental  action'  in  mixtures  of  two  or 
inore  sweetening  agents.  When  glucose  and  sucrose 
solutions  are  mixed,  for  example,  the  sweetness  of 
the  mixture  is  greater  than  would  be  predicted  by  the 
simple  addition  of  the  equivalent  sweetness  values  of 
each  component  stated  in  terms  of  the  equisweet 
sucrose  solution.  W  hen  such  mixtures  are  computed 
in  terms  of  the  equLsweet  glucose  concentrations, 
however,  the  sweetness  of  the  mixture  is  the  sum  of 
the  components.  There  is  simple  additixity  with  no 
supplementary  action  (44).  If  it  is  assumed  that  the 
magnitude  of  nerve  impulse  discharge  determines 
directly  the  magnitude  of  taste,  i.e.  sweetness,  we 
note  that  the  sensory  effect  for  glucose  is  nearly 
linearly  proportional  to  concentration,  but  for  sucrose 
it  is  cur\  ilinear,  i.e.  negatively  accelerated.  A  graphi- 
cal solution  of  the  addition  of  0.2  m  glucose  and  0.2 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


527 


sue   a  GLUC_ 

100- 

UJ 

to 

z 

0 

^"^ •SUCROSE 

a. 

/      ^---^ 

<n 

/'  ^^^"^ 

80- 

^>^ 

u. 

y'                                                              a  GLUCOSE 

SO- 

0 

/ol  B                              / 

UJ 

0 

3 

/        /                                                       .-                     0  MALTOSE 

K 

/       /                                       /'             '-'' 

40- 

H 
0 

•/     /          y 

< 

20- 

/ 

/            y' 

"^tJ.^                ,--                                                DISCHARGE 

MOLAR     CONCENTRATION 

0  — 

1       1       1        1        1        1        1        1        1       1 

value  to  match  .34  sucrose.  There  is  no  supplemental 
action  by  this  computation.  The  apparent  supple- 
mental action  with  one  set  of  transformations  but  not 
the  other  is  due  to  the  attempt  to  add  arithmetically 
one  linear  to  one  nonlinear  function.  This  example, 
although  derived  theoretically  from  the  electrophysio- 
logical response  curves,  can  be  matched  almost 
exactly  by  empirical  data  from  psychophysical  ex- 
periments on  man  (44).  Further  study  of  taste  mixtures 
by  the  electrophysiological  method  is  desirable.  The 
additive  analysis  presented  in  figure  16  is  theoretical 
except  that  the  response  curves  for  the  individual 
sugars  are  based  upon  experimental  points. 


BEH.^VIOR.^L    EFFECTS 


FIG.  16.  Response  of  rat  chorda  tympani  nerve  to  different 
concentrations  of  different  sugars.  The  dashed  line  SUC.  & 
GLUC  is  the  summated  response  to  be  e.xpected  when  sucrose 
solutions  are  added  to  0.2  M  glucose.  See  text  for  discussion. 
(From  Hagstrom,  E.  C,  unpublished  observations.) 


M  sucrose  solutions  in  a  mixture  can  be  made  from 
figure  16  by  adding  the  sucrose  curve  to  the  response 
of  0.2  M  glucose  at  A,  so  obtaining  the  dotted  line. 
The  total  sensory  effect  of  the  mixture  should  be  the 
sum  of  the  two  functions  at  point  B.  B  equals  62 
units,  a  magnitude  of  nerve  discharge  that  could  be 
produced  either  by  .34  sucro.se  or  .94  glucose,  indi- 
vidually. The  empirical  match  to  the  original  mixture 
can  be  stated  as: 


.2G  +  .2S  =  .34S 


CO 


In  sucrose  equisweet  solutions  where  .2G  =   .04.S,  the 
equation  (/)  becomes 


.04S  -|-  .2S  =  (sucrose  match) 


C^). 


But  since  the  arithmetic  sum  of  .04S  -\-  .2S  is  .24 
and  not  .34  (a  difference  of  .10)  there  is  supplemental 
action.  That  is,  the  empirical  match  shows  a  stronger 
sucrose  concentration  than  could  be  predicted  by 
the  simple  addition  of  equisweet  sucrose  solutions 
(i.e.  the  mixture  is  sweeter).  Setting  equation  (/)  in 
terms  of  glucose  where  .2S  =  .74G  we  have: 

.2G  -(-  .74G  =  .94G  (and  .94G  =  .34S). 

The  arithmetic  sum  of  .2G  -|-  .74G  equals  exactly 
.94G  which  is   the  same  as  the  equivalent   glucose 


Taste  stimulation  is  most  directly  related  to  food 
taking  and  the  rejection  or  avoidance  of  noxious 
stimulation.  Hence,  the  manifold  of  four  basic  tastes 
of  salt,  sour,  bitter  and  sweet  can  be  reduced  to  two 
behavioral  classes,  acceptance  01  rejection.  Certain 
substances  are  rejected  in  all  concentrations;  others 
may  be  accepted  at  low  but  rejected  at  high  concen- 
trations; still  others  may  be  accepted  at  all  concen- 
trations. Acceptance  or  rejection  may  also  be  in- 
fluenced by  postingestion  effects,  by  the  metabolic 
condition  of  the  organism  and  by  past  conditioning 
or  learning.  Thus  taste  is  only  one  of  several  determi- 
nants of  appetitive  behavior. 

Richter  (177)  in  his  classical  studies  of  self-selection 
demonstrated  that  the  animal's  behavior  was  a  part 
of  "'the  total  adjustive  mechanism  working  toward  the 
constancy  of  the  internal  en\ironment.  "  The  adrena- 
lectomized  rat  will  increase  the  intake  of  salt  solution 
to  such  a  degree  that  it  not  only  survives  but  gains 
weight.  It  is  significant  that  the  albino  rat  under 
normal  conditions,  i.e.  when  not  salt  hungry  or 
hormone  deficient,  displays  a  striking  preference  for 
sodium  chloride  and  other  sodium  salts  (15,  176,  203). 
This  behavior  is  exaggerated  after  adrenalectomy  as 
evidenced  by  a  lowered  preference  threshold  and 
greater  intake  of  all  salt  solutions  above  threshold 
in  preference  to  water.  An  excess  of  .salt  in  the  diet 
reduces  the  preference  and  may  even  wipe  it  out, 
leaving  only  an  aversion  over  the  entire  stimulus 
range.  Figure  1 7  summarizes  these  behavioral  phe- 
nomena together  with  a  plot  of  the  magnitude  of  the 
afferent  nerve  discharge  of  the  chorda  tsmpani  nerve 
in  the  rat.  The  solid  line  curves  show  the  relative 
preference  in  per  cent  (cc  salt  'cc  salt  -|-  cc  H2O) 
under  four  conditions:  adrenalectomized,  normal  (jV), 


528 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


with  5  per  cent  extra  salt  in  diet  and  lo  per  cent 
extra  salt.  The  basic  preference-aversion  response 
(TV)  is  shifted  systematically  by  these  changes  in 
salt  need.  In  the  normal  animal,  the  preference 
threshold  lies  above  the  concentration  at  which  a 
discharge  of  nerve  impulses  can  be  detected.  In  the 
adrenalectomized  rat  the  preference  and  electro- 
physiological thresholds  are  more  nearly  equal.  The 
normal  animal  thus  appears  to  taste  the  salt  but  does 
not  ingest  it.  The  adrenalectomized  rat  takes  the 
salt  solution  when  he  can  taste  it  (165). 

The  neural  response  curve  of  the  receptor  appears 
to  be  a  stable  properly  of  the  taste  bud.  The  threshold, 
i.e.  a  minimum  concentration  necessary  to  elicit  a 
discharge  of  the  taste  receptors,  is  essentially  the  same 
in  normal  and  adrenalectomized  rats  (166).  This  has 
been  confirmed  in  studies  using  the  conditioned  reflex 
method  (.15,  98).  Thus  the  change  in  self-selection 
behavior  cannot  be  explained  by  a  peripheral  change 
in  sensitivity  of  the  taste  receptors.  Of  similar  import 
is  the  finding  of  the  constancy  of  the  chorda  tympani 


response  in  insulin  hypoglycemia  (167).  Insulin  in- 
jection typically  leads  to  a  striking  increase  in  the 
preference  for  sugar  solutions  in  a  free  choice  situa- 
tion. 

Richter  has  presented  evidence  that  the  compensa- 
tory increase  in  intake  fails  when  the  sensory  nerves 
to  the  tongue  are  surgically  removed  (175).  The 
attenuation  in  preference  behavior  in  the  normal 
animal  after  combined  chorda  tympani-ninth  nerve 
deafferentation  further  supports  the  view  that  taste 
stimulation  triggers  the  response  to  taste  solutions 
(162).  A  rat  with  an  esophageal  fistula  will  show  the 
salt  preference  even  when  the  .solution  does  not  enter 
the  stomach  and  cannot  ha\e  a  metabolic  effect  (192). 
At  the  same  time  the  ingestion  of  water  or  salt  solu- 
tions can  be  modified  by  stomach  loading  by  intuba- 
tion with  sodium  chloride  solutions  which  'by-passes' 
taste,  but  the  effect  is  less  than  when  the  same  amount 
of  .solution  is  taken  by  mouth  so  that  the  taste  recep- 
tors are  stimulated  (132).  Thus  both  taste  and  intra- 
gastric factors  may  influence  drinking  C204). 


% 

100. 


80. 


60. 


40- 


20- 


0  r 
-3 


o  es 


-8.0 


-1.5 


-1.0 


-  .5 


1 

-2 

NoCI    LOG      M     Cone. 


"T" 
0 


FIG.  17.  A  composite  graph  of  the  neural  response  (broken  line)  and  the  preference  curves  (solid 
lines')  for  different  concentrations  of  NaCI.  The  neural  response  curve  (ordinate  to  the  right)  shows 
the  magnitude  of  the  electrical  activity  in  the  chorda  tympani  nerve  of  a  normal  rat.  Each  open 
circle  is  a  measure  of  the  integrated  electrical  activity  (in  milliamperes,  ma.)  of  the  discharge  at 
each  concentration.  The  preference  curves  were  obtained  from  four  different  groups  of  animals: 
adrenalectomized  (adren.),  normal  (.V),  normal  with  5  per  cent  additional  salt  in  the  diet  (j'/c) 
and  normal  with  10  per  cent  additional  salt  in  the  diet  (/o%),  respectively.  Each  point  is  the  av- 
erage preference  (or  aversion)  indicated  as  a  percentage  (cc  intake  salt)  -f-  (cc  salt  -|-  cc  H-.O)  at 
each  concentration.  Each  point  is  the  average  consumption  for  a  48-hr.  period  when  both  water 
and  salt  solution  were  continuously  available.  Salt  solutions  were  presented  in  ascending  order  of 
concentration.  [From  Pfaflfmann  (165).] 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


529 


The  role  of  taste  or  other  head  receptor  stimulation 
can  be  uncovered  if  the  postingestion  factors  can  be 
eliminated  or  minimized.  In  the  brief  exposure  be- 
havioral test  which  permits  little  ingestion,  rats  show 
a  preference  for  the  higher  of  two  concentrations  of 
sugar  solution  over  a  wide  range  of  pairs.  But  such 
equally  accepted  solutions  are  not  equally  ingested 
in  continuous  drinking  periods  as  brief  as  20  min.  in 
which  the  higher  concentrations  are  usually  con- 
sumed in  lesser  amounts  (204).  McCleary  (^142)  has 
clearly  demonstrated  the  role  of  intragastric  osmotic 
pressure  in  this  effect. 

In  another  behavioral  test,  the  Skinner  box  with 
sugar  solutions  as  reinforcers,  the  rate  of  bar  pressing 
on  an  aperiodic  reinforcement  schedule  is  faster,  the 
higher  the  concentration  of  sugar  (87).  This  schedule 
provides  relatively  little  drinking  per  response  and 
apparently  minimizes  postingestion  factors.  Further- 
more, the  concentrations  of  two  different  sugars, 
glucose  and  sucrose  found  to  give  equal  rates  of  re- 
sponse, i.e.  to  have  equal  reinforcing  value,  corre- 
spond to  the  equally  preferred  concentrations  in  the 
short  exposure  test  and  to  the  equally  sweet  concen- 
trations of  these  sugars  for  man.  Thus,  the  direct 
sensory  taste  effect  appears  not  only  to  instigate  in- 
gestion but  to  be  capable  of  reinforcing  the  acquisi- 
tion of  other  responses  leading  to  ingestion.  The 
usual  measures  of  intake  obscure  the  relation  to 
sensory  stimulation  because  of  postingestion  effects. 

The  nutritional  consequences  of  sugar  stimulation 
do  not  appear  to  be  essential  for  such  reinforcement. 
Nonnutritive  saccharin  solutions  can  also  serve  as 
reinforcers  for  the  acquisition  of  a  maze-running 
habit  (189).  The  degree  of  reinforcement  appears  to 
be  correlated  with  the  amount  of  consummatory  be- 
havior elicited.  Whether  reinforcement  power  is  de- 
termined by  the  magnitude  of  the  afferent  excitation 
per  se  or  by  the  magnitude  of  the  consummatory  be- 
havior elicited,  is  not  yet  clear. 

The  fact  that  certain  taste  stimuli  control  ingestion 
directly  appears  to  be  biologically  determined,  for 
nearly  all  organisms  accept  sugar  solutions  (78). 
Although  there  is  evidence  that  direct  injection  of 
nutrient  sugar  into  the  blood  stream  may  serve  as  a 
reinforcer  of  learning,  there  is  no  evidence  that  the 
"sweet  tooth'  depends  upon  the  concomitant  nourish- 
ment. The  drinking  of  nonnutritive  saccharin  solu- 
tions shows  no  sign  of  extinction  which  would  be  ex- 
pected if  the  preference  for  saccharin  were  acquired 
by  the  association  of  the  sweet  taste  with  nourishment 

(189). 

The  aversion  to  certain  stimuli  like  bitter  appears 


to  be  relatively  unmodifiable  by  experience.  In  one 
experiment,  guinea  pigs  two  days  post  jiartum  were 
provided  with  a  nontoxic  but  normally  avoided 
solution  as  the  only  source  of  fluid  until  three  weeks 
of  age.  This  substance  has  an  extremely  bitter  taste 
for  man.  Following  this  early  exposure,  preference 
tests  showed  that  the  avoided  stimulus  had  been 
rendered  somewhat  more  palatable,  but  in  a  retest 
three  months  later  the  effect  had  dissipated  so  that 
there  was  no  difference  between  the  control  and  ex- 
perimental animals.  The  aversion  had  not  been 
moderated  by  the  early  experience  (168). 

Thus  the  factors  that  control  behavior  in  the  taste 
preference  and  related  situations  are  becoming 
clearer.  Taste  may  trigger  ingestion  behavior  but 
alone  does  not  accoimt  for  it.  In  many  instances,  feed- 
ing behavior  is  directed  toward  the  physiological 
well-being  of  the  organisms;  but  situations  and 
habits  may  exist  which  are  contrary  to  the  physiologi- 
cal well-being.  According  to  Young,  "New  habits 
tend  to  form  in  agreement  with  bodily  needs,  but 
established  habits  tend  to  persist  as  regulators  of 
food  selection  even  when  the  food  selections  are  out 
of  line  with  bodily  needs."  The  limitations  of  self- 
selection  are  well  documented  (85,  loi,  204). 

Certain  of  these  basic  principles  appear  to  be  valid 
for  man.  Instances  of  enhanced  salt  craving  reported 
by  Richter  included  the  case  of  a  small  boy  who 
apparently  compensated  for  adrenal  insufficiency 
with  an  excessive  intake  of  table  salt  (177).  Patients 
in  whom  hypoglycemia  had  been  produced  for 
therapeutic  reasons  were  reported  to  find  strong 
sugar  solutions  more  palatable  than  when  blood  sugar 
levels  were  normal  (141).  The  bizarre  taste  cravings 
of  pregnant  women  are  well-known.  The  change  is 
not  one  of  taste  sensitivity  but  one  of  changed  likes 
and  di.slikes  (97).  The  metabolic  disequilibria  of 
diabetic  patients  are  often  associated  with  strong 
cravings  for  sweet,  although  to  satisfy  this  would 
run  counter  to  the  individual's  well-being  (i  77).  Thus, 
although  metabolic  changes  may  be  important  factors 
in  determining  the  hedonic  value  of  a  taste  stimulus, 
such  changes  do  not  always  automatically  lead  to 
self-corrective  behavior. 

The  well-known  fact  that  human  subjects  can  taste 
certain  substances  when  injected  intravenously  ap- 
pears to  support  the  view  that  taste  sensiti\ity  can 
be  influenced  by  constituents  in  the  blood  stream 
(102).  As  already  noted,  neither  the  adrenalectomized 
nor  hypoglycemic  animal  shows  evidence  of  a  change 
in  taste  sensitivity  when  studied  electrophysiologically. 
In  another  study  with  this  method,  preliminary  re- 


530 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


+  100^ 


!  +  50^ 


S-50^ 


-100^ 


Stimulus  Concentration 


FIG.  i8.  Preponderance  of  pleasant'  or  unpleasant'  judg- 
ments in  relation  to  the  concentration  of  taste  solution.  Ordmale 
gives  per  cent  'pleasant'  minus  per  cent  unpleasant.'  The 
abscissa  is  proportional  to  the  concentration,  the  full  length  of 
the  base  line  standing  for  40  per  cent  cane  sugar,  i .  1 2  per 
cent  tartaric  acid,  10  per  cent  NaOl  and  0.004  per  cent  quinine 
sulphate  (all  by  weight).  [From  Engel,  as  reproduced  in 
Woodworth  (200).] 


suits  showed  no  impulse  discharge  following  intra- 
venous injection  of  taste  stimuli  (Beidler,  L.  M., 
personal  communication).  Further  electrophysiologi- 
cal studies  of  'intravenous  taste'  are  called  for. 

Sherrington  (190)  has  noted  that  stimulation  of  the 
contact  sense  organs  often  initiates  a  chain  of  responses 
culminating  in  consummatory  behavior.  Taste  along 
with  tactile  stimulation  of  the  mouth  leads  directly 
to  the  retention,  chewing  and  swallowing  of  food  or 
its  expulsion.  Strong  affective  or  hedonic  tone  ap- 
pears to  he  a  basic  property  of 'nonprojicient'  receptor 


stimulation  as  compared  with  the  more  neutral  conse- 
quences  of  distance   receptor  stimulation. 

Troland  (196)  divided  receptor  stimulation  into 
classes  of  innate  biological  utility.  These  were 
nociception,  associated  with  deleterious  agents;  bene- 
ception,  with  stimuli  of  biological  utility;  and  neutro- 
ception,  with  stimuli  of  relatively  neutral  character. 
Different  tastes  might  fall  in  either  the  beneceptor 
or  nociceptor  classes.  These  three  classes  correspond 
closely  to  the  neutral,  unpleasant  or  pleasant  affectiv-e 
lone  aroused  by  sensory  stimulation  (202). 

Intensity  as  well  as  taste  quality  is  a  determiner  of 
hedonic  rating  as  shown  in  figure  18  (73).  Quinine 
is  mostly  unpleasant  and  is  increasingly  so  with  in- 
crease in  stimulus  concentration.  Sucrose  is  mostly 
pleasant  but  acid  and  salt  are  intermediate,  showing 
a  rise  in  pleasantness  to  a  maximum  and  then  a  fall 
with  increase  in  concentration.  These  hedonic  curves 
appear  to  resemble  the  preference  cur\es  found  in 
animal  studies  (.see  fig.  16),  particularh  for  salt  and 
quinine.  Hedonic  ratings  of  more  complex  tastes  and 
flavors  can  be  obtained  with  human  subjects  by 
means  of  rating  scales,  paired  comparison  judgments 
and  other  similar  tests.  These  have  had  wide  practical 
application  in  the  food  industry  and  the  armed  forces 
for  assessing  the  palatability  of  food  and  rations.  Such 
ratings  can  be  reliable  predictors  of  the  actual  accep- 
tance in  the  field  (171). 

Acceptability  of  food  by  man,  of  course,  is  deter- 
mined not  alone  by  taste.  Food  habits,  cultural  con- 
ditioning, immediate  social  pressures  or  other  com- 
plex psychological  factors  play  a  significant  role  in 
acceptability.  Acceptability  is  not  a  property  of  food. 
The  acceptance  of  food  is  a  response  of  the  organism, 
and  taste  as  one  component  in  flavor  may  play  an 
important  role  in  determining  this  response. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Abbott,    P.   S.    T/ie  Effect  of   Temperature  on    Taste  in  the 
While  Rat  (Thesis).  Providence,  R.  I. :  Brown  University, 

'953- 

2.  Abrahams,  H.,  D.  Kr.^k.auer  and  K.  M.  D.allenbach. 
Am.  3.  Psychol.  49:  462,   1937. 

3.  Adler,  a.  ^Isckr.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.   149:  208,   1934. 

4.  Allara,  E.  Arch.  ital.  anat.  e  embriol.  42 ;  506,   1 939. 

5.  Allara,  E.  Riv.  bid.  44:  209,   1952. 

6.  Allen,   F.   and   M.   Weinberg.   Quart.  J.  Exper.   Physiol. 

15:385.  I925- 

7.  Allen,  W.   F.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.   35;  275,    1923. 

8.  Allison,  J.  B.  j'.  Gen.  Physiol.   15:  621,   1932. 

9.  Allison,  J.    B.    and   W.    H.    Cole.   J.    Gen.    Physiol.    17: 
803,  1934. 

10.  Andersson,  B.,  S.  Landgren,  L.  Ols.son  and  '\'.  Zotter- 
MAN.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.   21:   105,    1950. 


11.  .\rev,   L.   B.,   M.  J.   Tremaine   and   F.    L.   Monzingo" 
Anat.  Rec.  64:  9,   1935. 

12.  Bacharach,  E.  Z^schr.  Biol.  84;  335,   1926. 

13.  Bacshaw,  M.  H.  and  K.  H.  Pribram.  J.  .Neurophvsiol .  16: 

499.  '953- 

14.  B.aradi,  a.   F.   and  G.   H.   Bourne.  .Xalure,  London    168: 

977.  ■95"- 

15.  Bare,  J.   K.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  42:  242,    1949. 
r6.  Beattv,  R.  M.  .\nd  L.  H.  Cragg.  J.  Am.  Chem.  Soc.  57: 

■■!347.  '935- 

17.  Beebe-Center,  J.   G.  .\ND  D.   VVaddell.  J.  Psychol.  26: 

5' 7.  ■948. 

18.  Beidler,   L.   M.  J.  .\europhysiol .    16:  595,    1953. 

19.  Beidler,  L.  M.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  38:  133,   1954. 

20.  Beidler,    L.    M.    In;    Chemistry   oj  .\atural   Food   Flavors, 
edited  by  J.  H.  Mitchell,  Jr.,  N.  J.  Leinen,  E.  M.  Mrsik 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


531 


and  S.  D.  Bailey.  Chicago,  III. :  Quartermaster  Research  63. 

and  Engineering  Command,    1957,  p.   7-  64. 

21.  BeIDLER,    L.    M.,    I.    Y.   FlSHM.\N   .\ND   C.    W.    Hardim.^n. 

Am.  J.  Physiol.  181:  235,   1955.  65. 

22.  Belehradek,  J.  Biol.  Rev.  5:  30,   1930.  66. 

23.  Benjamin,  R.  M.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  48;  119,  1955.  67. 

24.  Benjamin,  R.  M.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  48:  502,  1955.  68. 

25.  Benjamin,    R.    M.    and    C.    Pfaffmann.   J.   .Xntrophysiol. 
18:56,  1955. 

26.  Berlatzky,    a.    and   T.    Guevara.    Compl.    rend.    Soc.   de 

biol.  98:   176,   1928.  69. 

27.  Biester,  a.,  M.  W.  Wood  and  C.  S.   Wahlin.   .im.  J. 
Physiol.  73:  387,    1925.  70. 

28.  Blakeslee,    a.    F.    Proc.   Nat.    Acad.   Sc,    Washington^    18: 

120.   1932.  71- 

29.  Blanksma,  J-  J-  Rec.  Irav.  chim.  65:  203,   1946. 

30.  Bornstein,  VV.  S.   y'ale  J.  Biol.  &  Med.    13:   133,   1940. 

31.  Bremer,  F.  Arch,  internal,  physiol.  21:  308,  1923.  72. 

32.  BujAS,  Z.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.   119;  716,   1935.  73. 
33-  BujAS,  Z.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.   119:  835,   1935.  74- 

34.  BujAS,  Z.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.   119:   1360,   1935.  75. 

35.  BujAS,  Z.  Acta  Inst.  Psychol.,  Univ.  ^agreb  2(1):   18,   1937.  76. 

36.  BujAS,  Z.  Acta  Inst.  Psychol.,  Univ.  .^agreb  2(4);   12,   1937. 

37.  BujAS,  Z.  Acta  Inst.  Psychol.,  Univ.  .^agreb  3(3):   1,   1939-  77. 

38.  BujAS,  Z.  Annee  Psychol.  50:  159,   1949.  78. 

39.  BujAS,  Z.  Acta  Inst.  Psychol.,  Univ.  Z^S'^^  '7-   '°'   '953-  79- 

40.  BujAS,  Z.  AND  A.  Chweitzer.  Annee  Psychol.  35:  147,  1934.  80. 

41.  BujAS,   Z.   AND  A.   Chweitzer.   Compt.   rend.  Snc.   de  biol.  81. 
126:  1 106,  1937.  82. 

42.  BujAS,    Z.    and    A.    OsTOjcic.    Acta    Inst.    Psychol.,    Univ.  83. 
Zagreb  3:  I,  1939. 

43-   BujAS,    Z.    AND    A.    OsTOjcic.    Acta    Inst.    Psychol.,    Univ.  84. 
Zagreb  13:  i,  1941. 

44.  Cameron,  A.  T.  Scientific  Report  Series  No.  g.  New  York :  85. 
Sugar  Research  Foundation,    1947. 

45.  Carr,  W.  J.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  43:  377,   1950.  86. 

46.  Chinaolia,  L.  Riv.  psicol.   11:  196,   1916.  87. 

47.  Cohen,  J.  and  D.  P.  Ogdon.  Science  no:  532,   1949.  88. 

48.  Cohen,  J.  and  D.  P.  Ogdon.  Psychol.  Bull.  46:  490,  1949. 

49.  Cohen,  M.  J.,  S.   Hagiwara  .-^nd  Y.   Zotterman.   Acta  8g. 
physiol.  scandinav.  33;  316,   1955. 

50.  Cohen,  M.  J.,  S.  Landgren,  L.  Strom  and  Y.  Zotter-  90. 
man.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  40:  Suppl.   135,  1957.  91. 

51.  CoHN,  G.  Die  organischen  Geschmacksstoffe.  Berlin:  Siemen- 

roth,  1914.  92- 

52.  Cole,  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  20:  602,  1910.  93. 

53.  Cole,  W.  H.  and  J.  B.  Allison.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.   14:  71,  94. 

1930- 

54.  Cole,  W.  H.  and  J.  B.  Allison.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  15:119,  95, 

'93'- 

55.  Cole,  VV.  H.  and  J.  B.  .Allison.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  16:  895, 

1933-  96- 

56.  Corin,  J.   Arch.   Biol.   Pans  8:    121,    1888.  97. 

57.  Cox,  G.  J.  and  J.  W.  Nathans.  J.  .-ippl.  Physiol.  5:  395, 

1953-  98- 

58.  Cragg,   L.  H.    Tr.  Roy.  Soc.  Canada  31(3):  7,    1937. 

59.  Cragg,  L.  H.   Tr.  Roy.  Soc.  Canada  31(3):  131,   1937.  99- 

60.  Crozier,  W.   ].  J.  Comp.  Neurol.   26:    i,    1916.  100. 

61.  Crozier,  W.  J.  J.  Comp.  .Veurol.  26:  453,  191 6.  loi. 

62.  Crozier,  \V.  J.  In:  Handbook  of  General  Experimental  Psy- 
chology, edited  by  C.  Murchison.  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.  102. 
Press,  1934,  p.  987.  103. 


Cushing,  H.  C.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hasp.   14:  78,   1903. 
Dallenbach,  J.  VV.   and  K.   M.   Dallenbach.  Am.  J. 
Psychol.  56:  21,    1943. 

Dethier,  V.  G.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  35:  55,   1951. 
Dethier,  V.  G.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   165;  247,   1951. 
Dethier,  V.  G.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  30:  348,   1955. 
Dethier,    V.    G.    In:    Molecular    Structure    and    Functional 
Activity  in  the  Nerve  Cell,   edited   by   R.   G.   Grenell   and 
J.   L.   Mullins.   Wa.shington:  American   Institute  of  Bio- 
logical Sciences,  1956 
Dethier,  V.  G.  and  L.  E.  Chadwick.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  33: 

589.  1950- 

DoDT,  E.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  26: 

346,  1952- 

Dzendolet,    E.    Intensity-duration   Relations  for    Taste   using 

Electrical  Stimulation  (Thesis).   Providence,   R.    I. :   Brown 

University,  1957. 

Ectors,   L.  Arch,   internal,  physiol.   43:   267,    1936. 

Engel,  R.  Arch.  ges.  Psychol.  64:  i,  1928. 

Fabian,  F.  W.  and  H.  B.  Blum.  Food  Res.  8:  179,   1943. 

Fishman,  I.  Y.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  49:  319,   1957. 

Fodor,  K.  .^ND  L.  Happisch.  .Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  197:  337, 

1922. 

FoLE-i',  J.  .\.  Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  60:  262,  1945. 

Frings,  H.   Turtox  News  24:  No.  8,   1946. 

Frings,  H.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  41  :  25,   1948. 

Gavda,  T.  Arch,  fisiol.    10:   175,    1912. 

Gerebtzoff,  M.  a.  Cellule  48:  91,   1939. 

Gerebtzoff,  M.  a.  Arch,  intermit,  physiol.  51:   igg,   1939. 

Gibson,  L.  and  T.  Hartmann.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  30:  311, 

'9I9- 

Gley,   E.   and  C.   Richet.   Compl.   rend.  Soc.   de  biol.   742: 

1885. 

Gordon,  J.  G.  and  D.  E.  Tribe.  Brit.  J.  Anim.  Behav.  2: 

72,  1954- 

GoUDRI.«iN,  J.  C.  Arch,  neerl.  physiol.    15:  253,    1930. 

Guttman,  N.  J.  Comp.  <S  Physiol.  Psychol.  47:  358,    1954. 

Hagstrom,    E.    C.    Doctoral   Thesis.    Providence,   R.    I. ; 

Brown  University,  1957. 

H.^gstrom,  E.  C.  AND  C.  Pfaffmann.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol. 

Phychol.  In  press. 

Hahn,  H.  Ixlm.    U'chnschr.    11:   1504,   1932. 

Hahn,  H.  and  Gunther,  H.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  231  :  48, 

1932. 

Hahn,  H.  ^teAr.  Sinnesphysiol.  65:    105,    1934. 

Hahn,  H.  hlin.    Wchnschr.    15:  933,   1936. 

H.^hn,  H.  Beitrage  zur  Reizphysiologie.  Heidelberg:  Scherer, 

■949- 

Halpern,  B.   p.  Electrical  .Activity  in  the  .Medulla  Oblongata 

following  Chemical  Stimulation  of  the  Rat's   Tongue  (Thesis). 

Providence,  R.  I.:  Brown  University,   1957. 

H.Xnig,  D.   p.  Phil.  Stud.    17:  576,   1901. 

Hansen,  R.   and  W.   Langer.  Klin.    Wchnschr.    14:   11 73, 

'935- 

Harriman,  a.   E.  and  R.  B.   MacLeod.  Am.  J.  Psychol. 

66:465,  1953. 

Harris,  H.  Nature,  London  163:  878,   1949. 

Harris    H.  and  H.  Kalmus.  Ann.  Eugenics  15:  24,   1949. 

Harris,  L.  J.,  J.  Clay,  F.  J.  Hargreaves  and  .\.  Ward. 

Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  ser.  B  113:  161,   1933. 

Hartridge,  H.  J.  Physiol.   103;  34,   1945. 

Harvey,  R.  B.  J.  Am.  Chem.  Soc.  42:  712,   1920. 


532 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH^SIOLOGV 


NEUROPHVSIOLOnV    I 


104.  Hasler,  a.  D.  and  J.  A.  Larsen.  Scient.  Am.  ig^j:  72,  1955.  144. 

105.  HoBER,  R.,  D.   I.  Hitchcock,  J.  D.  Baterman,  D.  R. 
GoDDARD    AND    VV.    O.    Fenn.    Physical   Chemistry   of  Cells  145. 
and  Tissues.  Philadelphia:  Blakiston,   1945.  146. 

106.  HoBER,  R.  AND  F.  Kiesow.  ^Ischr.  physik.  Chem.  27:  601, 

1898.  147. 

107.  HoLWAY,  A.  H.  and  L.  M.  HuRVif:n.   Am.  J.  Psychol.  49:  148. 

37.  '937- 

108.  HoLWAY,  A.  H.   and  L.   M.   HiRVicH.  J.  E.xper.  Psychol.  149. 

23:  191,  1938.  150. 

109.  Hopkins,  A.  E.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  61;  13,   193J. 

I  10.   Hopkins,  J.  W.  Cnnad.  J.  Res.  24F:  203,    1946.  151. 

111.  Janowitz,  H.  D.  and  M.  I.  Grossman.  J.  Appl.  Physiol.  152. 
2:  217,  1949. 

112.  Jones,  M.  H.  Am.  j.  Psychol.  67:  696,    1954.  153. 

113.  Jones,  M.  H.  and  F.  M.  Jones.  Science  115:  355,   1952. 

114.  Kahlenberg,  L.  J.  Phys.  Chem.  4:  33,   1900.  154. 

115.  Kaplick,  M.  .^Ischr.  ^ellfotsch.  u.  mikroskop.  .Inal.  38:  57', 

1953-  '55- 

116.  Kenrick,  F.  B.   Ti.  Roy.  Soc.  Canada  25:  227,   1931. 

117.  Keppler,  F.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  1:  449,   1869. 

118.  Kiesow,  F.  Phil.   Stud.   10:  329,   1894.  156. 

119.  K.IESOW,  F.  Phil.  Stud.  10;  523,   1894. 

1 20.  Kiesow,  F.  Arch.  ital.  biol.  30:  377,   1898.  157. 

121.  KiMURA,   D.   .AND   L.    M.    Beidler.    .4m.  J.    Phvuol.     187: 

610,  1956.  158. 

122.  Kionka,  H.  and  F.  Stratz.  .irch.  exper.  Path.  u.  Phar-  159. 
makol.  95:  241,  1922.  160. 

123.  Knowles,  D.  and  p.  E.  Johnson.  Food  Res.  6;  207,  1941.  161. 

124.  Kolmer,   W.    In:   Handbuch   der    Mikroskopischen   Anatomic 

des  Menschen,  edited  by  W.  Mollendorf.  Berlin:  Springer,  162. 

1927.  P-  '54-  '63. 

125.  KoMURO,  K.  Arch,  neerl.  physiol.  5:  572,   1921.  164. 

126.  KoPERA,  A.  Arch.  ges.  Psychol.  82:  273,   1931.  165. 

127.  Krakauer,  D.  and  D.  M.  Dallenbach.  Am.  J.   Psychol.  166. 

49:  469.  '937- 

128.  Landgren,  S.  .Acta  physiol.  scandtnav.  40:  210,    1957.  167. 

129.  Lasareff,  p.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  194:  293,   1922. 

1 30.  Lashley,  K.  S.  Brain  .Mechanisms  and  Intelligence.  Chicago :  1 68. 
Univ    Chicago  Press,   1929. 

131.  Lehm.an,  a.  J.  .1.  Food  &  Drug  Officials  U.  .S'. ,  Qiiarl.  Bull.  169. 
14(3):  82,  1950.  170. 

132.  Le  Magnen,  J.  J.  Physiol.  Paris,  47:  405,   1955.  171. 

133.  L.EMBERGER,  F.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  123:293,  1908.  172. 

134.  Lewis,  D.  R.  J.  Psychol.  26:  437,   1948. 

135.  Liljestrand,  G.  Arch,  neerl.  physiol.  7:  532,    1922.  173. 

136.  Liljestrand,  G.  and  Y.  Zotterman.  Acta  physiol.  scan-  174. 
dinav.  32:  291,   1954.  175. 

137.  MacLeod,  S.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  44:  316,   1952.  176. 

138.  MacNichol.  E.  F.,  Jr.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  H.  K.  177. 
Hartline.  XIX  Internat.  Physiol.  Congr.,  Ahsir.  of  Com-  1 78. 
mumc:  1953. 

139.  Maruhashi,  J.,  K.  MizuGUCHi  .\KV>  I.  Tas.aki.  J.  Physiol.  179. 
117:  129,  1952. 

140.  Mayer,   B.   ^tschr.   Psychol.   Physiol.  Smnesorg.  .iht.   11  58:  180. 

'33.  1927- 

141.  Mayer-Gross,  W.  and  J.  VV.  Walker.  Brit.  J.  Exper.  181. 
Path.  27:  297,   1946. 

142.  McCleary,    R.    .\.  J.   Comp.  &   Physiol  P.tychol.   46:   411,  182. 

1953- 

143.  McFadden,  H.  J.   Psychol.   4:  349,    1937.  183. 


Moncrieff,  R.  W.   Ihe  Chemical  Senses.  New  York:  Wiley, 

1951- 

MoRUZi,  A.  and   Lechtinski.    Rev.  neurol.    70:  478,   1938. 

Mountcastle,  V.  and   E.  Henneman.  J.  .Veurophysiol.    12: 

85.  '949- 

Oehrwall,    H.    Skandmav.    .irch.    Physiol.    11:    245,    1901. 

Oertly,   E.    and   R.    G.   Myers.  J-   -l"'-   Chem.   Soc.   41: 

855.  I9'9- 

Olmsted,  J.  M.  D.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  31:  465,  1920, 
Oppel,  .\.  In :  Lehrbuch  der  lergleichende  mikroskopischen 
Anatomic  der  Wirbeltieren.  Jena:  Fischer,  1900,  vol.  III. 
Ostwald,  W.  and  a.  Kuhn.  Kolloid-^tschr.  29:  266,  1921. 
Parker,  G.  H.  Smell,  Taste  and  Allied  Senses  m  the  Verte- 
brates. Philadelphia:  Lippincott,  1922. 
P.\rker,  G.  H.  .and  C.  B.  Metcalf.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  17: 
55,  1906-07. 

Parker,  G.  H.  and  E.   M.  Stabler,  .-int.  J.  Physiol.  32 : 
230,  1 91 3. 

P.\TTON,    H.    D.    In:     Tfxlhook    of    Physiology    (17th    ed.), 
edited    l>v    J.    F.    Fulton.    Philadelphia:   Saunders,    1955, 

P-  377- 

Patton,  H.  D.  and  V.  E.  Amassi.an.  J.  .Xeurophysiol.   15: 

245.  '952- 

Patton,   H.    D.,   T.    C.    Ruch   and   .\.    E.    Walker.   J. 

.Xeurophysiol.  7:   171,   1944. 

PAUL-Mi.iNCHEN,  T.  ^tschr.  Elektrochem.  28:  435,   1922 

Penfield,  VV.  AND  E.  Boldrey.  Brain  60:  389,   1937. 

Pf.affm.ann,  C  j'.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.   17:  243,   1941. 

Pfaffmann,    C.    In:   Handbook  of  Experimental  Psychology, 

edited  by  S.  S.  Stevens.  New  York:  Wiley,  1951,  p.  1143. 

Pfafpm.\nn,  C.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  45:  393,  1952. 

Pfaffmann,  C.  Science  117:  470,   1953. 

Pfaffmann,  C.  J.  .\europhysiol.   18:  429,   1955. 

Pfaffmann,  C.  Am.  J.  Clin.  Nutrition  5:   142,   1957. 

Pfaffmann,    C.    and  J.    K.   Bare.   J.    Comp.  &    Physiol. 

Psychol.  43:  320,   1950. 

Pf.aff.vlann,   C.   and    E.    C.    H.'\GsrROM.    .im.  J.    Physiol. 

183:  651,  1955. 

Pfaffmann,  C.  .\nd   R.   P.  W.arren.  J.  .Appl.  Psychol.  In 

press. 

PiERON,  H.  Annee  Psychol.  20:  42,    191 4. 

Pierrel,   R.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  49:  374,    1955. 

Pilgrim,  F.  J.  ^4;;;.  J.  Clin.  Nutrition  5:   171     1957. 

Ranson,   S.    W.    and   S.    L.    Clark.    The  Anatomy  of  the 

Nervous  System.  Philadelphia:  Saunders,   1955,  p.  232. 

Rengvist,  Y.  Skandmav.  Arch.  Physiol.   38:  97,    191 9. 

Richards,  T.  W.  Am.  Chem.  J.  20:   121,    i8g8. 

Richter,  C.   p.    Tr.  Am.  Neurol.  A.  65:  49,    1939. 

RiCHTER,  C.   P.  Endocrinology  24:  367,   1939. 

Richter,  C.  P.  Harvey  Lectures  38:  63,   1942. 

RicHTER,   C;.   P.   Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  63:  364, 

1946. 

Richter,  C.  P.  and  K.  H.  Campbell.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  128: 

291,  1940. 

Richter,  C.   P.  and  K.   H.  Clisby.  .\m.  J.  Physiol.    134: 

157.  '94>- 

Richter,  C.   P.   and  .\.  MacLean.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.    126: 

■•  1939- 

Rose,  J.   E.   and  V.   B.   Mountcastle.    J-   Comp.  .\eurol. 

97:  441,  1952. 

Ross,  S.   and  J.  Vers.ace.  .Am.  J.  Psychol.  66:  496,    1953. 


THE    SENSE    OF    TASTE 


533 


184.  RowiNSKi,  Di,  p.  AND  G.  Manunta.  Arch.  fisiol.  53:   117, 

1953- 

185.  Saidullah,  a.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  60:  457,   1927. 

186.  ScHUTZ,  H.  G.  AND  F.  J.  Pilgrim.  J.  E.xper.  Psychol.  54: 

41.  1957 

187.  ScHUTZ,  H.  G.  AND  F.J.  Pilgrim.  Food  Res.  22:  206,  1957. 

188.  Schwartz,  H.  G.  and  G.  Weddell.  Brain  61  :  99,   1938. 

189.  Sheffield,  F.   D.   and  T.  B.   Robv.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol. 
Psychol.  43:  471,    1950. 

190.  Sherrington,    C.     7 he    Integrative    Action    of    the    .\ervous 
System.  New  Haven:  Yale  Univ.   Press,    1948. 

191.  Shore,  L.  E.  J.  Physiol.  13:  191,  1892. 

192.  Stellar,  E.,  R.  Hvman  and  S.  .S.amet.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol. 
Psychol.  47:  220,   1954. 

193.  Stevens,  S.  S.  Psychol.  Rev.  64:  153,   1957. 

194.  Taylor,  H.  W.  Protoplasma  4:   i,   1928. 

195.  Taylor,  H.  W.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.   11:  207,   1928. 


1 96.  Troland,    L.   T.    The   Fundamentals   of  Human   Motivation. 
New  York:  van  Nostrand,   1928. 

197.  VON    Frisch,    K.    In:    Handbuch   der   .\ormalen    und   Patho- 
logische  Physiologic  11:  203,   1926. 

198.  von  Skramlik,  E.  In:  Handbuch  der  Physiologic  der  Niederen 
Sinne  1:7,!  926. 

199.  Warren,  H.  C.  and  L.  Carmichael.  Elements  of  Human 
Psychology.  Boston:  Houghton  Mifflin,   1930,  p.   122. 

200.  VVoodvvorth,    R.    S.    (editor).    Experimental    Psychology. 
New  York:  Holt,    1938,  p.  498. 

201.  WooLSEY,    C.    H.    AND   D.    H.    LeMessurier.    Fed.    Proc. 

7:  137.  1948. 

202.  Young,   P.  T.   Motivation  of  Behavior.  New  York:  Wiley, 
1936. 

203.  Young,  P.  T.  Comp.  Psychol.  Monogr.   19:  No.  5,   1949. 

204.  Young,  P.  T.  Am.  J.  Clin.  Nutrition  5:  154,  1957. 

205.  Zotterman,  Y.  Skandinav.  Arch.  Physiologic  72:  73,   1935. 

206.  Zotterman,  Y.   .icta  physiol.  scandinav.  37;  60,    1956. 


CHAPTER    XXI 


The  sense  of  smell 


\V.    R.     A  D  E  V     \     Department  nf  Anatomy,  University  of  Melbourne,  Melbourne,  Australia 


CHAPTER    CONTENTS 

Olfactory  Mucosa  and  Peripheral  Receptor 
Arrangement  of  C31factory  Mucosa 

Olfactory  Bulb  and  Its  Connections  with  Olfactory  Mucosa 
Essential  Processes  Involved  in  Olfactory  Stimulation 
Characters  of  Odorous  Substances 
Enzyme  Theories  of  Olfaction 
Radiation  Theories  of  Olfaction 
Methods  of  Odor  Measurement 

Subjective  measurement  techniques 
Objective  measurement  techniques 
Electrical   Phenomena   in   Olfactory   Bulb  Accompanying   Ol- 
factory Stimulation 
Patterns  of  Spontaneous  and  Induced  Activity  in  Bulb  and 
Effects  of  Anesthesia 
Spontaneous  waves 

Waves  and  unit  activity  accompanying  olfactory  stimula- 
tion 
Differential  Excitation  of  Receptors 
Differentiation  of  response  in  area 
Temporal  differentiation  of  response 
Central  Connections  of  Olfactory  Bulb 
Efferent  Pathways  From  Olfdctory  Bulb 
Extent  of  Primary  Olfactory  Cortex 
Neuroanatomical  investigations 
Electrophysiological  investigations 
Higher  Order  Olfactory  Connections 
Behavior  Studies  of  Olfactory  Mechanisms 


At  alt  stages  of  corticat  etaboratio7i  an  important  function  of 
ttie  olfactory  cortex,  in  addition  to  participation  in  its  own 
specific  way  in  cortical  associations,  is  to  serve  as  a  non-specific 
activator  Jor  all  cortical  activities. 

C.   J.    HERRICK,     1933 

WHILE  THE  IMPORTANCE  of  the  olfactory  sense  is 
greatly  reduced  in  primates  in  comparison  with  other 
telereceptor  mechanisms,  such  as  sight  and  hearing, 
it  can  nevertheless  provide  significant  information 
about  events  possibly  remote  in  space  and  time.  In- 


deed, olfaction  may  provide  the  only  warning  of 
serious  environmental  hazards,  and  in  man  it  retains 
its  importance  in  feeding  and  sexual  functions. 
There  remain  many  baffling  aspects  to  even  the 
most  basic  phenomena  in  the  olfactory  process,  par- 
ticularly in  the  mechanisms  ins-olved  in  excitation  of 
the  peripheral  receptor  and  in  the  physiological 
patterning  of  activity  through  which  fine  differences 
in  odors  are  presumably  perceived. 


OLF.\CTORV    MUCOS.\    AND    PERIPHER.XL    RECEPTOR 

Arrangement  nf  Oljactory  Mucosa 

The  olfactor\-  mucosa  forms  a  restricted  zone  in 
man,  lying  in  the  dorsal  and  posterior  part  of  the 
nasal  cavity.  To  the  naked  eye  it  appears  yellowish- 
brown  in  comparison  with  the  rest  of  the  mucosa, 
and  it  covers  the  upper  parts  of  both  the  lateral  wall 
of  the  nasal  cavity  and  septum,  extending  over  a 
total  area  of  about  240  sq.  mm  (66).  The  olfactory 
mucosa  is  a  pseudostratified  columnar  epithelium 
and,  unlike  the  respiratory  portion,  has  no  distinct 
basement  meinbrane  or  cilia.  It  lines  the  surface  of 
nearly  all  the  superior  turbinate,  a  small  part  of  the 
middle  turbinate  and  the  upper  third  of  the  nasal 
septum  (fig.  i). 

Inspired  air  traverses  the  inferior  meatus  and 
partially  the  middle  meatus  during  normal  breath- 
ing, and  the  olfactory  area  is  thus  above  the  main 
air  current.  Since  a  change  in  breathing,  as  in  sniffing, 
causes  adequate  eddying  of  air  into  the  upper  olfac- 
tory area,  it  is  apparent  that  aerodynamic  factors 
may  be  intimately  concerned  in  determining  thresh- 
olds of  excitability  (56). 

The  olfactory  receptors  or  hair  cells  are   bipolar 


535 


53^ 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FR.  S. 


SPH.  S. 


FIG.  I.  Diagrammatic  representation  of  the  lateral  wall  of 
the  nasal  cavity,  indicating  the  general  extent  of  the  olfactory 
epithelium  (dotted).  The  main  stream  of  inspired  air  passes 
below  the  olfactory  region.  Secondary  eddying  of  air  currents 
carries  odors  to  the  receptor  region.  Abbreviations:  CR.PL., 
cribriform  plate,  perforated  by  the  olfactory  nerves,  FR.S., 
frontal  air  sinus;  I.T.,  .\I.T.,  S.T.,  inferior,  middle  and  su- 
perior turbinate  bones  projecting  as  ledges  from  the  lateral 
wall  of  the  nasal  cavity;  SPH.S.,  sphenoidal  air  sinus. 


and  oval.  When  seen  by  light  microscopy,  they  pos- 
sess distally  small  terminal  swellings  from  each  of 
which  five  to  six  olfactory  hairs  commonly  arise.  In 
the  rabbit,  these  cells  are  estimated  to  number  150,000 
per  sq.  mm  (35).  Electronmicroscopy  indicates  that 
the  hairs  are  considerably  more  numerous,  with  up 
to  1000  hairs  per  cell  (23).  Each  hair  is  i  to  2  /:i  long 
and  o.i  /a  in  diameter.  In  this  way  the  surface  area  ex- 
posed by  the  receptor  cell  is  greatly  increased  (fig.  2). 
Clark  (32)  points  out,  however,  that  near  the  pe- 
riphery of  the  olfactory  epithelium  there  is  some 
intermingling  of  olfactory  receptors  and  ciliated 
epithelial  cells  of  the  'respiratory'  region,  and  that 
this  may  lead  to  misinterpretations  in  electronmicros- 
copy of  the  olfactory  hairs,  which  he  considers  to 
be  coarser  and  to  remain  untapered  at  their  free  ex- 
tremities. 

Removal  of  the  olfactory  bulb  in  the  raljljit  pro- 
duces a  very  striking  degeneration  in  48  hr.,  with 
almost  complete  removal  of  the  debris  of  the  mucosal 
receptors  within  three  days  (33).  However,  only 
about  half  the  receptors  degenerate  after  complete 
removal  of  the  bulb,  the  remainder  persisting  un- 
altered up  to  six  months  after  operation.  The  findings 
do  not  support  the  concepts  that  the  axons  of  the 
residual  elements  may  proceed  to  adjacent  areas  of 
the  olfactory  epithelium,  rather  than  to  the  ijulb,  or 


that  they  may  give  off  collaterals  sufficient  to  main- 
tain the  cells.  The  possibility  is  considered  of  a  cen- 
trifugal system  of  fiijers  arising  in  the  bulb  and 
proceeding  peripherally  to  the  mucosa. 

The  secretions  of  the  numerous  serous  and  mucous 
glands  in  both  the  respiratory  and  olfactory  regions 
of  the  nose  bathe  the  entire  cavity  in  a  liquid  sheath 
which  is  in  a  constant  state  of  motion  towards  the 
nasopharynx.  This  sheath  may  be  of  basic  importance 
in  conveying  odorous  substances  to  the  receptor  cell, 
since  varying  degrees  of  water  and  fat  solubility  in 
the  tissues  of  the  mucosa  may  be  related  to  the  odorous 
properties  of  a  particular  substance  (see  below). 

The  electrical  responses  of  the  olfactory  mucosa  of 
the  frog  have  been  successfully  recorded  by  Otto.son 
(76).  Odorous  air  blown  into  the  nasal  cavity  evokes 
a  slow  negative  monophasic  potential  in  the  olfactory 
mucosa  (fig.  3).  The  response  is  obtained  only  from 
the  olfactory  area  of  the  mucosa  and  is  not  abolished 
by  cocaine  in  concentrations  sufficient  to  paralyze 
olfactory  nerve  fibers.  It  is  abolished  i^y  small  amounts 
of  ether  or  chloroform  vapor,  and  it  is  inferred  that 
the  response  arises  in  the  olfactory  hairs.  The  ampli- 
tude of  the  respon.se  is,  within  certain  limits,  propor- 
tional to  the  logarithm  of  the  stimulus  intensity. 
Equal  amounts  of  odorous  material  distributed  in 
different  volumes  of  air  evoke  responses  of  equal 
amplitudes.  The  shape  and  time  course  of  the  re- 
spon.se  is  related  to  the  strength  of  the  stimulus.  With 
an  increase  of  odor  intensits'  in   the  stimidating  air. 


HG.  2.  Electronmicrograph  of  the  surface  of  a  human  ol- 
factory cell,  showing  a  great  number  of  thin  finger -like  processes 
1.5  to  2.0  11  long.  Magnification  X  23,430.  [From  Bloom  & 
Engstrom  (23).] 


THE    SENSE    OF   SMELL 


537 


FIG.  3.  Rhytlimic  waves  superimposed  upon  the  slow  potential.  Stimulus;  amy]  acetate.  Volume 
of  stimulating  air,  0.25  cc.  Vertical  line  5.0  mv.  Time  bar,  i  sec.  [From  Ottoson  (76).] 

FIG.  4.  Rhythmic  waves  evoked  by  continuous  stimulation  of  the  olfactory  epithelium.  Stimulus: 
0.1  mole  butanol.  Velocity  of  stimulating  air  stream,  i.o  cc  per  sec.  Vertical  line  i.o  mv.  Time  bar 
0.5  sec.  [From  Ottoson  (76).] 


the  potential  rises  at  a  faster  rate,  the  crest  of  the 
response  broadens  and  the  decay  time  lengthens.  The 
'wave  form'  of  the  stimulating  air  current  is  of  great 
importance  in  determining  the  shape  and  time  course 
of  the  response.  The  latency  of  the  response  to  stimu- 
lation with  butanol  of  different  stimulus  strengths 
varies  from  0.2  to  0.4  sec. 

Ottoson  has  found  that  during  continuous  stimu- 
lation the  evoked  response  in  the  olfactory  epithelium 
declines  from  the  initial  peak  to  a  lower  level  which 
continues  throughout  stimulation  (fig.  4).  The  ampli- 
tude of  this  residual  response  is  lower  at  higher  stimu- 
lus intensities.  With  repeated  stimulation  at  short 
intervals,  the  first  three  or  four  responses  are  pro- 
gressively diminished,  with  greater  reductions  at 
higher  stimulus  strengths.  The  sensitivity  of  the 
epithelium  to  different  substances  can  be  selectively 
reduced  by  repetitive  stimulation,  and  rhythmic 
oscillations  are  often  seen  on  the  peak  of  the  slow 
response  evoked  by  strong  stimuli. 

Olfactory  Bulk  and  Its  Conrnrllons  with  Olfactvry  Mucosa 

The  olfactory  nerve  fibers  arising  from  the  hair 
cells  penetrate  the  overlying  cribriform  plate  of  the 
ethmoid  bone  and  enter  the  olfactory  bulb.  Electron- 


microscopy  of  the  olfactory  nerves  indicates  a  unique 
appearance,  with  large  numbers  of  very  small  nerve 
fibers  having  a  modal  diameter  of  0.2  n.  They  num- 
ber six  million  from  one  side  of  the  nasal  septum  in 
the  pig  and  are  considerably  more  numerous  from 
the  turbinates  (45).  There  appears  to  be  a  one-to- 
one  relationship  between  receptor  cells  and  axons. 
Their  conduction  velocity  in  the  pike  is  0.2  m  per 
sec,  thus  resembling  the  last  elevation  in  the  C  fiber 
action  potential  in  the  frog's  .sciatic  nerv-e. 

Within  the  outer  layers  of  the  bulb  the  fibers  of  the 
olfactory  nerves  enter  into  the  formation  of  glomeruli 
(fig.  5).  Each  glomerulus  is  formed  jointly  by  enter- 
ing olfactory  nerve  fibers  and  also  from  the  dendrites 
of  more  deeply  situated  mitral  and  tufted  cells.  These 
cells  forrn  the  succeeding  second  order  neurons  on 
the  olfactory  pathway.  The  arrangement  is  an  excel- 
lent one  for  spatial  summation,  since  each  glomerulus 
in  the  rabbit  receives  impulses  from  26,000  receptors 
and  passes  this  information  through  24  mitral  cells 
and  68  tufted  cells  (15,  16).  The  axons  of  the  60,000 
mitral  cells  form  the  bulk  of  the  lateral  olfactory 
stria  passing  to  higher  olfactory  centers. 

Physiological  and  anatomical  evidence  confirms  the 
existence  of  a  regional  projection  pattern  from  the 
olfactory   mucosa    to    the    bulb.    Impulses   from    the 


538 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


OLF.  N.F. 


FIG.  5.  General  arrangement  of  the  neural  paths  in  the  ol- 
factory bulb.  Fibers  from  the  receptor  cells  are  collected  on 
the  surface  of  the  bulb  (OLF .„V.F.)  and  participate  in  the  for- 
mation of  more  deeply  situated  glomeruli  iGLOM')  which 
also  receive  the  dendritic  processes  of  the  mitral  cells  (^W)  and 
the  tufted  cells  (T).  Axons  of  mitral  cells  are  mainly  collected 
into  the  lateral  olfactory  tract  QLAT.  OLF.  TR.')  and  run  to 
the  primary  olfactory  cortex.  The  finer  axons  of  tufted  cells 
pass  into  the  anterior  limb  of  the  anterior  commissure,  reach- 
ing the  opposite  bulb  where  they  synapse  with  deeply  situated 
granule  cells  (G).  .■\xons  of  granule  cells  are  directed  peripher- 
ally at  least  as  far  as  the  fields  of  the  mitral  and  tufted  cells. 
OLF.  VEjVT.  represents  the  olfactory  ventricle  present  in 
lower  mammals  and  continuous  with  the  cerebral  ventricular 
system. 


anterior  and  dorsal  parts  of  the  olfactory  mucosa 
reach  the  anterior  parts  of  the  iaulia,  whereas  the 
ventral  and  posterior  reE;ions  of  the  mucosa  project 
to  the  posterior  parts  of  the  bulb  (5).  Although  initial 
anatomical  studies  (35)  did  not  support  a  topographic 
arrangement,  more  extensive  investigations  (31)  have 
confirmed  the  general  arrangement  suggested  bv 
Adrian.  The  upper  part  of  the  olfactory  epithelium 
projects  mainly  to  the  upper  part  of  the  bulb,  and 
lower  areas  of  epithelium  to  lower  regions  of  the 
bulb;  but  anatomical  evidence  of  an  anteroposterior 
organization  of  the  projections  is  less  definite. 


ESSENTIAL    PROCESSES    INVOLVED    IN 
OLFACTORY    STIMULATION 

Characters  nf  Odorous  Substances 

No  embracing  picture  can  yet  be  advanced  to 
categorize  all  odorous  substances,  since  many  are 
totally  unrelated  physically  and  chemically.  Hill  & 
Carothers  (54)  observed  a  relationship  between  the 
number  of  atoms  in  certain  macrocyclic  ring  hydro- 


carbon compounds  and  the  nature  of  their  odors. 
Thus  compounds  with  13  atoms  possess  a  cedar-like 
odor,  with  14,  15  or  16  atoms  a  musk-like  odor,  and 
with  17  or  18  atoms  a  civet-like  odor.  For  example, 
pentadecanolide  and  decamethylene  oxalate  both 
have  musky  odors. 


-CO 


(CH,)i4 


(CHOi, 


Pentadecanolide 


O 


-  O 

I 

CO 

! 

CO 

I 

-  o 


Decamethvlene  oxalate 


Hill  &  Carothers  suggest  that,  within  wide  limits, 
the  number  of  atoms  in  the  ring,  rather  than  the 
identity  of  the  reactive  groups,  is  the  significant  fac- 
tor. However,  many  anomalies  have  limited  attempts 
to  extend  this  hypothesis.  Moncrieff  (68)  has  sug- 
gested that  to  be  odorous  a  substance  must  be  volatile 
and  soluble  in  the  tissues  of  the  olfactory  mucosa,  the 
latter  property  involving  varying  degrees  of  water 
and  fat  solubility.  The  olfactory  mucous  membrane 
exhibits  considerable  powers  of  adsorbing  odorous 
substances  in  the  freshly  isolated  state,  and  this 
sorptive  property  may  be  intimately  related  to  proc- 
esses of  excitation  in  olfactory  receptors  (69).  Mon- 
crieff further  suggests  that  the  disposition  of  an  ele- 
ment in  the  electrochemical  series  may  be  correlated 
with  odorous  properties,  since  only  seven  elements 
are  odorous  (fluorine,  chlorine,  bromine,  iodine, 
oxygen  as  ozone,  phosphorus  and  arsenic),  and  six 
of  these  occupy  the  lowest  places  in  the  electrochemi- 
cal series.  The  disposition  of  substitution  groups  in 
organic  compounds  is  of  great  significance  in  deter- 
mining both  strength  and  quality  of  odors.  Legge 
(64)  has  advanced  the  hypothesis  that  odorous  sub- 
stances may  react  with  groups  on  the  free  surface  of 
protein  and  lipoprotein  films,  leading  to  the  rupture 
of  a  few  bonds  in  incompletely  spread  proteins  with 
a  consequent  enorinous  increase  in  their  area.  In  view 
of  the  role  played  by  — S — S —  groups  in  the  main- 
tenance of  protein  structure,  the  rupture  of  bonds 
induced  by  odorous  substances  might  explain  the 
high  dilutions  at  which  mercaptans  can  be  detected. 

On  the  basis  of  records  from  the  olfactory  bulb, 
.\drian  (10)  has  defined  four  groups  of  .substances, 
and  in  each  group  has  detected  one  substance  capable 
of  evoking  a  discharge  limited  to  a  single  one  of  the 
units   within    the   range   of  the   electrodes.    Acetone 


THE    SENSE    OF    SMELL 


539 


exhibits  a  high  specificity  in  a  group  which  includes 
amyl  and  ethyl  acetate.  Benzene  behaves  similarly 
in  a  group  composed  of  aromatic  hydrocarbons. 
Octane  is  similarly  active  in  a  group  of  paraffin 
hydrocarbons  and  heavy  oils.  Dipentane,  cedarvvood 
oil  and  eucalyptus  oil  (substances  belonging  to  the 
terpenes  and  related  compounds)  likewise  give  single 
unit  discharges. 

The  ability  of  various  substances,  including  metal 
surfaces,  to  adsorb  and  retain  foreign  odors  has  been 
tested  by  Deininger  &  Sullivan  (36).  The  great  ma- 
jority of  metal  surfaces  not  only  pick  up  odors,  but 
also  modify  and  distort  them,  often  so  severely  as  to 
leave  little  suggestion  as  to  the  original  contaminant. 
This  perversion  of  the  perceived  odor  is  not  related 
to  the  purity  of  the  metal  in  the  case  of  either  copper 
or  aluminum. 

Enzyme  Theories  of  Olfaction 

Alexander  (11)  has  suggested  that  odor-producing 
substances  affect  the  catalyst  balance  of  the  olfactory 
cells.  This  theory  has  been  elaborated  by  Kistiakow- 
sky  (60)  in  an  hypothesis  that  substances  having  odor 
inhibit  a  reaction  requiring  a  catalyst.  These  changes 
in  the  concentration  of  reaction  products  would 
cause  excitation  in  specific  receptor  units.  Sumner 
(81)  has  criticized  this  concept  on  the  grounds  that 
substances  we  smell,  in  the  concentrations  needed  to 
smell  them,  would  not  be  likely  to  have  any  effect 
on  any  known  enzyme  systems  and  would  require  an 
array  of  enzymes  with  new  and  unusual  properties 
in  the  olfactory  mucosa.  Beidler  (21)  points  out  that 
some  substances  are  eff^ective  olfactory  stimuli  in 
concentrations  as  low  as  io~''  molar. 

Even  if  an  odorous  substance  should  inactivate  an 
enzyme,  thus  causing  a  change  in  concentration  of 
certain  substances,  there  is  as  yet  no  explanation  as 
to  how  this  change  in  concentration  could  stimulate 
olfactory  nerves.  Bourne  (26)  and  El-Baradi  &  Bourne 
(38,  39}  have  detected  significant  amounts  of  alkaline 
phosphatase  in  the  olfactory  mucosa  and  in  the  taste 
buds  of  the  tongue  and  have  observed  that  vanillin 
inhibits  this  alkaline  phosphatase.  However,  alkaline 
phosphatase  is  widely  distributed  throughout  the 
body.  Goldwasser,  quoted  by  Sumner  (81),  suggests 
that  the  energy  needed  to  stimulate  olfactory  recep- 
tors may  come  from  Pauling's  electrochemical  energy 
source  deriving  from  the  modification  of  bonding 
angles  within  a  molecule  at  the  time  that  the  molecule 
goes  into  solution. 


Radiation  Theories  of  Olfaction 

Theories  of  electromagnetic  radiation  or  molecular 
viljration  in  relation  to  olfaction  have  engaged  luany 
workers  (20,  37,  74,  84,  90).  However,  there  appears 
to  be  little  or  no  experimental  foundation  for  the 
concept  that  the  essential  properties  of  odor  result 
from  radiations  inherent  in  molecular  behavior  (73). 
Indeed,  a  suljstance  such  as  the  deuteroxyl  counter- 
part of  n-butyl  alcohol  has  exactly  the  same  odor  as 
the  original  «-lnit\l  alcohol,  although  its  infrared 
adsorption  spectrum  is  different  (89).  On  the  other 
hand,  certain  (^/-  and /-isomers  diflfer  in  smell,  although 
their  infrared  spectra  are  identical.  No  evoked  elec- 
trical responses  can  ije  recorded  from  the  olfactory 
mucosa  if  it  is  covered  with  a  thin  plastic  membrane 
which  transmits  infrared  radiation  but  impedes  con- 
tact between  the  stimulating  particles  and  the  epi- 
thelium. There  is  no  indication  that  olfactory  re- 
ceptors can  be  stimulated  unless  the  odorous  material 
is  brought  into  contact  with  the  epithelium  (76). 

Methods  oj  Odor  Measurement 

SUBJECTIVE      ME.ASUREMENT      TECHNIQUES.      SourCCS      of 

error  in  the  subjective  assessment  of  odor  quality 
and  intensity  have  been  discussed  in  a  historical  sur- 
vey by  Wenzel  (87).  It  is  obviously  difficult  to  con- 
trol such  factors  as  the  force  of  the  ob.server's  inhala- 
tion in  methods  invoking  the  ' sniff  technique,  nor 
does  the  administration  of  the  odorous  substance  by 
a  stream  of  air  at  constant  pressure  necessarily  con- 
trol mechanical  factors  in\'olved  in  the  eddying  of 
air  towards  the  olfactory  receptors  in  the  upper  part 
of  the  nasal  cavity. 

Only  a  few  of  the  many  subjecti\e  methods  will  be 
discussed  here,  since  all  appear  to  involve  significant 
possibilities  of  error  in  assessment  of  threshold,  and 
there  are  conflicting  opinions  as  to  their  relative 
merits.  A  number  of  early  methods,  typified  by  the 
olfactometer  of  Zwaardemaker  (90),  involved  sniffing 
gradually  increasing  intensities  of  the  odorous  sub- 
stance up  to  threshold  concentration.  In  attempts  to 
obviate  subjective  sniffing,  both  injection  of  a  blast 
of  air  and  a  continuous  stream  of  air  have  been  tested 
by  Elsberg  &  Levy  (40).  They  defined  the  absolute 
olfactory  threshold  as  the  minimal  blast  of  odorous 
air  capable  of  producing  a  sensation  of  odor.  Although 
the  measurement  so  obtained  is  usually  expressed  in 
terms  of  volume,  Jerome  (55)  has  suggested  that  these 
threshold  measurements  are  dependent  on  pressure 
of  the  air  Ijlast  rather  than  on  odor  intensity.  Jones 
(56)   has   also   found    that   aerodynamic   factors,   es- 


540 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


pecially  pressure,  determine  the  threshold  in  the 
blast  injection  technique  and  that  the  threshold  so 
determined  is  not  related  to  odor  concentration. 

Since  neither  subjective  sniffing  nor  the  substitu- 
tion of  an  air  blast  for  the  observer's  own  sniff  dispose 
of  many  of  the  difficulties  in  odor  threshold  measure- 
ment, Wenzel  (88)  has  studied  the  reliability  of 
threshold  measurements  during  normal  breathing 
with  the  subject's  head  placed  in  a  camera  inodorata. 
Here  a  plastic  box  encloses  the  subject's  head,  and 
the  hair  and  face  can  be  covered  with  plastic  to 
eliminate  their  odors,  leaving  only  the  nostrils  ex- 
posed. A  continuous  stream  of  pure  air  to  which 
odors  can  be  added  is  fed  into  the  box.  This  method 
is  claimed  to  give  satisfactory  results  in  threshold 
measurement  without  the  need  for  prior  training  of 
the  subject. 

It  has  been  claimed  that  subjects  can  match  odors 
quantitatively  by  the  use  of  a  standard  "sniff'  tech- 
nique, using  as  test  substances  two  aliphatic  homol- 
ogous series,  comprised  of  the  alcohols  butanol  (CO 
through  duodecanol  (Cii),  and  the  acetate  esters 
hexyl  (Ce)  through  duodecyl  (C12),  each  prepared  in 
serial  dilutions  (19).  No  attempt  was  made  in  these 
tests  to  instruct  the  subject  in  the  technique  of  sniffing, 
reliance  being  placed  on  the  subject's  existing  habits. 
Results  were  consistent  in  repeated  trials,  with  odor- 
osity  decreasing  as  a  function  of  molecular  chain 
length,  and  also  with  dilution,  for  both  alcohols  and 
acetates. 

Kuehner  (61)  has  used  an  air-dilution  method  in 
determining  olfactory  thresholds  and  stresses  the 
need  to  standardize  the  subject  from  day  to  day  by 
exposing  him  to  standard  vapor  concentrations.  Ex- 
treme variations  of  sensitivity  noted  in  individuals 
make  it  impossible  to  take  odor  measurements  over 
extended  periods  without  knowledge  of  the  subject's 
sensitivity  at  the  time  of  sampling.  One  breath  of 
ammonia  can  reduce  the  sensitivity  by  50  per  cent 
for  24  hr.  At  the  onset  of  a  head  cold,  the  sensitivity 
is  sharply  increased  but  later  is  depressed.  A  marked 
temporary  reduction  in  sensitivity  follows  excessive 
drinking  or  smoking.  Kuehner  used  air  saturated 
with  xylene  vapor  as  a  reference  odor  and  in  a  pro- 
longed series  of  experiments  with  two  subjects  ex- 
hibiting similar  sensitivities  found  that  its  normal 
odor  level  was  380  times  greater  than  threshold. 
Under  the  same  conditions,  nicotine  saturated  air  to 
3000  times  the  threshold  intensity. 

OBJECTIVE    ME.\SUREMENT    TECHNIQUES.    Kuchner    (6l) 

claims  that  odorous  substances  are  capable  of  react- 


ina;  with  oxidants  and  that  the  resulting  deodoriza- 
tion  is  related  to  the  amount  of  oxidant  reduced. 
While  efforts  to  find  reproducible  concentrations  of 
individual  odors  in  a  complex,  such  as  tobacco  smoke, 
were  unsuccessful,  it  was  found  that  certain  oxidants 
react  with  odor  complexes  in  a  reproducible  manner, 
thus  confirming  the  earlier  observations  of  Lang  et  al. 
(62).  Kuehner  found  a  close  uniformity  in  odor  pro- 
duction by  tobacco  artificially  burned  in  this  system 
regardless  of  type,  freshness  and  rate  of  combustion, 
and  established  that  tobacco  smoked  by  humans 
produced  only  40  per  cent  of  the  odor  level  of  that 
'smoked'  artificially,  both  in  its  reducing  powers  with 
oxidants  and  as  determined  subjectively  under  room 
conditions.  This  technique  has  been  criticized  by 
Turk  (85)  on  the  grounds  that  cxidant  methods, 
which  usually  employ  permanganates  or  eerie  salts, 
may  be  inadequate  because  of  a  lack  of  relationship 
between  quality  or  intensity  of  odorants  and  their 
reactivity  toward  a  chemical  oxidant.  Such  methods 
may  indicate  only  the  reducing  or  oxidizing  qualities 
of  the  extraneous  atmospheric  gases  and  vapors. 

Turk  (85)  has  used  infrared  adsorption  spectra  of 
odorous  substances  as  a  means  of  qualitative  analysis. 
Different  functional  groups,  such  as  aldehyde,  alco- 
hol and  ester  linkages,  show  typical  infrared  adsorp- 
tion frequencies.  Thus  the  infrared  spectrum  of  a 
mixture  of  compounds  reveals  information  on  the 
types  of  individual  compounds  therein,  and  by  this 
means  Turk  has  been  able  to  detect  vaporized  mineral 
oil  as  a  component  of  apple  aroma  in  commercial 
apple  storage  rooms.  It  has  already  been  mentioned 
that  this  method  may  not  be  free  from  error,  since 
substances  with  similar  odors  may  have  different  ad- 
sorption spectra  and  vice  versa  (87,  89). 


ELECTRICAL   PHENOMEN.A   IN   OLF.ACTORY   BULB 
.ACCOMP.ANYING    OLF.ACTORY    STIMUL.ATION 

Patterns  of  Spontanrous  and  Induced  Activity  in  Bulb 
and  Effects  of  Anesthesia 

The  rhvthmic  waxes  that  can  be  recorded  from 
the  surface  of  the  bull)  have  been  extensively  investi- 
gated (4-10,  72,  86).  Whereas  Adrian's  initial  ex- 
periments suggested  rhythmic  discharges  in  the  bulb 
svnchronously  with  each  ijieath,  later  experiments 
(5,  9)  have  shown  that  with  filtered  air  no  mechanical 
stimulation  occurs.  Both  spontaneous  waves  and 
those  which  occur  in  response  to  strong  olfactory 
stimuli  have  been  recorded  in  the  bulb. 


THE    SENSE    OF   SMELL 


541 


FIG.  6.  Intrinsic  waves'  recorded  in  the  olfactory  bulb  in  light  thiopental  anesthesia.  A:  No  ol- 
factory stimulus.  Frequency,  100 /sec.  B:  Abolition  of  intrinsic  rhythm  by  weak  olfactory  stimulation 
with  amyl  acetate.  C:  In  another  preparation,  strong  stimulation  with  amyl  acetate  abolishes  the 
intrinsic  rhythm  with  substitution  of  a  slower  induced  rhythm.  [From  .\drian  (5).] 


SPONT.ANEnus  WAVES.  Thcsc  are  usually  smaller  and 
less  regular  than  waves  evoked  by  olfactory  stimula- 
tion and  are  associated  with  persistent  activity  in  the 
cells  of  the  bulb.  They  were  described  in  the  isolated 
olfactory  bulb  of  the  frog  (47).  Their  frequencies  are 
as  high  as  70  to  100  per  sec  (fig.  6).  They  are  sup- 
pressed in  the  mammal  by  deep  anesthesia  and  they 
accompany  a  persistent  irregular  discharge  of  axon 
spikes  in  the  deeper  layers.  In  medium  anesthesia 
this  discharge  may  be  so  large  as  to  conceal  any 
change  induced  by  a  weak  olfactory  stimulus.  This 
continuous  activity  persists  after  complete  destruction 
of  the  olfactory  epithelium  and  after  isolation  of  the 
bulb  from  the  forebrain  but  ceases  after  interference 
with  the  blood  supply  of  the  bulb. 

Adrian  (5)  regards  this  activity  as  largely  spon- 
taneous or  intrinsic,  expressing  the  continuous  break- 
down and  repair  of  cells  not  stabilized  by  deep  anes- 
thesia. Although  mitral  cells  certainly  take  part  in 
this  activity,  there  is  some  reason  to  suppose  that  it 
may  originate  in  cells  with  short  axons  (granule 
cells)  arranged  in  layers  deep  to  the  mitral  cells.  This 
is  suggested  by  the  fact  that  the  intrinsic  waves  can 
exhibit  considerably  higher  frequencies  than  the  in- 
duced waves.  If  the  induced  waves  indicate  the 
maximum  frequency  of  discharge  of  the  direct  olfac- 
tory pathwa\',  a  higher  rhythm  probably  has  a  dif- 
ferent origin. 

In  the  phase  of  recovery  from  deep  anesthesia,  the 
bulb  is  quiescent  unless  stimulated,  but  a  breath  of 
odorous  air  will  produce  a  few  waves  accompanied 
bv   .scattered    discharges    in    the    mitral    cells.    \Vith 


lightening  anesthesia  such  a  stimulus  may  start  a 
longer  train  of  waves  of  gradually  decreasing  fre- 
quency. Ultimately  a  stage  is  reached  at  which  the 
bulb  reacts  with  a  train  of  waves  which  may  con- 
tinue indefinitely.  This  phenomenon  has  been  named 
by  Adrian  the  '  awakening  reaction'  of  the  bulb,  and 
it  is  suggested  that  in  medium  or  light  anesthesia  the 
granule  cells  have  become  capable  of  maintaining 
themselves  in  continued  activity  and  that  their  ac- 
tivity leads  to  a  continued  discharge  in  the  mitral 
cell  axons. 

WAVES  AND  UNIT  ACTIVITY  ACCOMPANYING  OLF.^CTORY 

sTiMUL.-iiTiON.  In  such  animals  as  the  cat  and  rabbit, 
strong  olfactory  stimuli  elicit  large  sinusoidal  oscil- 
lations in  the  bulb,  usually  at  a  fixed  frequency  and 
occurring  only  with  each  period  of  stimulation  (fig.  7). 
These  large  regular  waves  are  produced  only  by 
olfactory  stimuli  given  at  three  or  four  times  the 
threshold  concentration,  and  may  appear  at  fre- 
quencies of  50  to  60  per  sec.  against  a  silent  back- 
ground in  moderately  deep  urethane  anesthesia,  and 
at  10  to  15  per  sec.  under  allobarbital  or  pentobarbi- 
tal. Their  frequency  does  not  bear  any  relationship 
to  the  quality  or  intensity  of  the  stimulus. 

In  moderately  deep  allobarbital  or  pentobarbital 
anesthesia,  with  the  bulb  exhibiting  regular  intrinsic 
waves  at  a  low  frequency,  an  olfactory  stimulus  usu- 
ally abolishes  the  waves  at  each  inspiration.  If  the 
stimulus  is  strong  the  gap  in  intrinsic  activity  may  be 
filled  with  induced  waves.  As  anesthesia  lightens,  the 
rhvthm    becomes   more   firmlv   established    and    the 


542 


HANDBOOK    OF    FHVSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7-  Induced  waves  develop  in  the  olfactory  bulb  after  commencement  of  the  olfactory  dis- 
charge. Rabbit  under  deep  urethane  anesthesia  breathing  air  containing  amyl  acetate.  The  upper 
oscillograph  tracing  shows  the  waves  from  the  surface  of  the  bulb;  the  lower  shows  the  axon  spikes 
from  the  white  matter.  Inspiration  indicated  by  white  line  above.  Time  marker,  o.  i  sec.  [From 
Adrian  (5).] 


waves  may  be  merely  reduced  or  scarcely  altered 
during  stimulation.  When  the  iaulb  is  quiet  in  very 
deep  anesthesia,  a  moderate  olfactory  stimulus  sets 
up  a  mitral  cell  di.scharge  with  each  inspiration,  but 
no  discharges  are  visible  between  inspirations.  With 
lightening  anesthesia,  evoked  discharges  appear 
against  a  background  of  continuous  irregular  activity 
which  ultimately  becomes  so  prominent  as  to  obscure 
entirely  any  change  evoked  by  the  stimulus.  In  very 
light  anesthesia  the  olfactory  stimuli  regain  some 
control  over  the  mitral  pathway,  and  both  weak  and 
strong  stimuli  evoke  an  obvious  increase  in  discharge 
during  each  inspiration,  with  suppression  of  the 
mitral  discharges  in  the  periods  between  each  in- 
spiration. 

The  complete  suppression  of  intrinsic  activity  in 
the  bulb  of  the  rabbit  is  seldom  long  maintained.  The 
return  of  activity  takes  place  more  slowly  when  the 
smell  is  strong  and  the  anesthesia  light.  Adrian  (5) 
suggests  that  the  phenomenon  offers  an  explanation 
of  olfactory  adaptation  as  seen  in  man,  although,  as 
mentioned  above,  records  from  the  mucosa  during 
continuous  stimulation  indicate  that  at  least  some 
adaptation  occurs  at  the  receptor  level  (76). 

Records  from  the  olfactory  bulb  in  man  show  a 
series  of  rhythmic  waves  at  each  inspiration  while 
breathing  tincture  of  valerian  and  benzene,  whereas 
room  air  yields  no  response.  No  spontaneous  waves 
of  the  type  seen  in  the  rabbit  have  been  noted  in 
man.  Thiopental  anesthesia  abolishes  all  responses 
(80). 

Unit  activity  recorded  with  microelectrodes  in  the 
olfactory  bulb  of  a  variety  of  animals  favors  the  mitral 
cells  as  the  site  of  origin  of  the  axon  spikes,  with 
tufted  cells  and  glomeruli  possibly  also  contributing. 
Where  it  is  possible  to  record  both  wave  and  spike 
components  of  the  response,  it  is  found  that  the  fast 
spikes  are  evoked  first,  followed  by  the  waves,  with 
the  spikes  becoming  synchronous  with  the  waves  as 
the  wave  response  develops  (5,  72). 


Differential  Excitation  of  Receptors 

DIFFERENTIATION     OF    RESPONSE    IN    AREA.     Substances 

soluble  in  water  (e.g.  amyl  acetate,  ethyl  acetate, 
ether,  acetone)  have  a  lower  threshold  for  spike  dis- 
charges in  the  anterior  part  of  the  bulb,  where  mitral 
cells  synapse  with  fibers  from  the  anterior  and  dorsal 
parts  of  the  mucosa  (fig.  8).  Conversely,  substances 
soluble  in  lipoids  (e.g.  pentane,  coal  gas  and  ben- 
zene) have  a  lower  threshold  for  spike  discharges  in 
the  posterior  part  of  the  bulb  which  receives  fibers 
from  the  posterior  and  ventral  parts  of  the  olfactory 
epithelium  (9,  10).  This  difference  does  not  neces- 
sarily imply  a  differential  excitability  of  the  receptors 
at  the  front  and  back  of  the  organ  and  may  well  re- 
sult from  structural  factors,  difference  in  the  velocity 
of  the  air  current  and  in  the  composition  of  the  sur- 
face film  through  which  molecules  of  odorous  sub- 
stance pass  to  reach  the  receptor  surface. 

In  records  from  the  middle  part  of  the  bulb  (9) 
there  may  be  a  single  series  of  large  spikes  or  a  mix- 
ture of  large  and  small  spikes  (fig.  9).  The  single 
series  presumably  represents  a  discharge  from  one 
cell,  whereas  small  spikes  come  from  neighboring 
units.  Adrian  found  that  at  any  one  recording  point 
one  substance  in  low  concentration  would  give  a 
single  series  of  large  spikes.  Each  large  spike  thus 
appears  to  have  a  special  relation  to  a  particular 
stimulus.  Units  have  been  observed  displaying  this 
specific  sensitivity  to  such  diverse  substances  as 
trimethylamine,  acetone,  ethyl  acetate,  amyl  acetate, 
pentane,  octane,  xylol,  petrol,  clove  oil,  oil  of  euca- 
lyptus and  thick  machine  oil.  Despite  the  improb- 
abilitv  of  finding  a  few  primary  smells  out  of  which 
all  others  can  be  compounded,  .Adrian  (lo)  has  de- 
fined four  groups  of  substances  with  one  substance  in 
each  group  most  frequently  evoking  a  single  unit 
discharge  (see  above).  Strong  concentrations  of 
odorants  will  bring  in  other  units,  but  critical  regions 
will    always   exist   where    the   concentration    is   only 


THE    SENSE    OF   SMELL 


543 


FIG.  8.  Three  records  with  double  oscillograph  system  showing  discharge  from  the  oral  and 
aboral  regions  of  the  rabbit's  olfactory  bulb.  In  each  record  the  upper  tracing  is  from  the  oral  region 
and  the  lower  from  the  aboral.  The  signal  line  shows  increasing  odor  concentration.  With  acetone 
(top  record)  the  discharge  is  confined  to  the  oral  region,  with  paraflFin  oil  (bottom  record)  to  the 
aboral  and  with  amyl  acetate  (middle  record)  discharge  occurs  in  both  regions.  [From  Adrian  (9).] 


just  great  enough  to  e.xcite  and  there  the  specific 
excitation  will  always  show  itself. 

TEMPORAL  DIFFERENTI.\TION  OF  RESPONSE.  Adrian  (lo) 

suggests  that  at  the  beginning  and  end  of  each  respira- 
tion the  concentration  of  odorous  substance  is  near 
threshold  values.  Physical  and  chemical  properties  of 
the  substance  will  therefore  determine  the  time  course 
of  the  response.  The  integrated  outline  or  envelope 
of  the  response  as  .seen  in  oscillographic  records  will 
thus  have  a  characteristic  contour  and  a  particular 
smell  might  be  identified  from  this  outline.  Volatility 
and  solubility  in  water  both  favor  a  rapid  rise  and 
decline  of  the  discharge,  with  little  pensistence  be- 
tween one  inspiration  and  the  next.  Thus  the  re- 
sponse to  amyl  acetate  has  a  shorter  latency  and  a 
more  abrupt  rise  and  fall  than  the  longer  latency 
responses  to  pentane.  Increasing  concentrations  affect 
the  areal  differentiation  but  have  no  effect  on  the 
temporal  pattern.  Patterns  of  temporal  integration 
have  been  recorded  i^y  the  more  elaborate  techniques 
of  Mozell  &  Pfaffmann  (72)  in  determining  the 
relative  sensitivity  of  different  parts  of  the  mucosa 
and  bulb  to  amyl  acetate  and  heptane  (fig.  10). 

Mozell  (71)  has  determined  the  neural  response 
curve  of  the  integrated  spike  discharge  from  four 
points  on  the  olfactory  bulb  as  a  function  of  concen- 
tration of  amyl  acetate,  heptane,  ethyl  ether  and 
benzene.  Discharge  strength  and  duration  increased 
approximately  as  a  negatively  accelerated  function  of 
concentration.  The  curves  reached  their  a.symptotes 
in  about  i  to  1.5  log  units  of  physical  concentration. 
By  contrast,  the  asymptote  in  other  senses  is  not 
reached  until  the  intensities  have  been  increased 
several  thousand  times,  or  bv  4  to  6  log  units.  It  is 


suggested  that  this  may  account  for  the  relatively 
narrow  range  of  suiyective  odor  intensity  discrimina- 
tion. This  study  confirms  the  existence  of  a  relatively 
gross  anteroposterior  spatial  differentiation  of  re- 
sponsive zones  within  the  bulb  for  different  substances. 
Thus  smells  seem  to  be  distinguished  by  a  combina- 
tion of  detailed  pattern  and  general  region  of  excita- 
tion (lo).  Hainer  et  al.  (52),  in  discussing  an  informa- 
tion theory  of  olfaction,  also  emphasize  the  iinportance 
of  threshold  phenomena  in  the  conveyance  of  essen- 
tial olfactorv  information. 


CENTR.'^L    CONNECTIONS    OF    OLF.ACTORY    BULB 

Olfactory  functions  were  originally  ascribed  to 
many  deep  parts  of  the  temporal  lobe,  including  the 
hippocampal  gyrus  and  hippocampal  formation,  and 
to  certain  regions  of  the  frontal  lobe,  including  the 
cingulate  area.  Much  of  the  early  work  in  this  field 
can  be  seen  in  an  English  translation  of  certain  works 
of  Ramon  y  Cajal  (78).  Experimental  determination 
of  the  sites  of  termination  of  the  olfactory  tract  has 
indicated  a  much  more  restricted  distribution  of  these 
fibers.  Reviews  by  Brodal  (27),  Allison  (15)  and 
Pribram  &  Kruger  (77)  have  discussed  the  morpho- 
logical aspects  of  this  problem. 

Efferent  Pathways  From  Olfactory  Bulb 

The  majority  of  the  axons  of  the  mitral  cells  run 
caudally  to  be  collected  on  the  lateral  and  inferior 
aspects  of  the  olfactory  peduncle,  forming  the  lateral 
olfactory  tract  or  stria.  In  addition  to  the  superficial 
pathways,  there  is  a  centrally  placed  group  of  delicate 


544  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOCn'    I 


•/^ 


K^<  /\c«SSUs. 


FIG.  9.  Records  from  the  middle  region  of  the  rabbit's  olfactory  bulb  showing  the  differential 
sensitivity  of  neighboring  mitral  units.  In  this  case,  acetone  gives  only  large  spikes;  amyl  acetate 
gives  large  and  small;  and  pentane  gives  only  small  spitces.  [From  .Adrian  (9).] 


ANTERIOR     BULB 


POSTERIOR     BULB 


AMYL  HEPTANE 

ACETATE 


FIG.  ID.  Integrator  records  of  the  spike  discharges  from  the 
anterior  and  posterior  areas  of  the  olfactory  bulb  following 
stimulation  by  heptane  and  amyl  acetate.  The  responses  indi- 
cate a  differentiation  in  both  space  and  time.  [."Kfter  Mozell 
&  Pfaffmann  (72).] 


axons  arising  mainly  from  the  tufted  cells  and  trav- 
ersing the  anterior  commissure  to  reach  the  opposite 
olfactory  bulb.  The  existence  of  a  medial  olfactory 
stria  conveying  fibers  from  the  bulb  to  the  septal 
area  appears  unlikely  (40-  Although  the  tuberculo- 
septal  tract  is  often  stated  to  conve\  impulses  from 
the  olfactory  tubercle  to  the  septum,  this  tract  arises 
largely  in  ihc  nonolfactorv  part  of  the  olfactor\ 
tubercle  (15). 


Extent  oj  Primary  Olfactory  Cortex 

NEUROA.N.ATOMiCAL  INVESTIGATIONS.  Removal  of  the 
olfactory  bulb  in  the  rabbit  (34)  and  monkey  (67)  is 
follovvfed  by  degeneration  of  fibers  running  in  the 
lateral  olfactory  tract  to  reach  the  olfactory  tubercle, 
the  frontal  prepyriform  cortex,  the  temporal  prepyri- 
form  cortex,  the  cortical  and  medial  amygdaloid 
nuclei  and  the  bed  nucleus  of  the  stria  terminalis. 
The  general  arrangement  of  these  structures  is  shown 
in  figure  1 1 .  An  essentially  similar  distribution  occurs 
in  the  marsupial  phalanger  (i).  No  degeneration  has 
Ijeen  seen  in  these  studies  in  the  hippocampal  forma- 
tion nor  in  the  posterior  pyriform  cortex  (entorhinal 
area),  nor  is  there  evidence  of  a  medial  olfactory 
tract  establishing  direct  cingulate  or  septal  connec- 
tions. 

Following  inoculation  of  the  olfactory  mucosa  of 
rhesus  monkeys  with  poliomyelitis  virus,  Bodian  (25) 
has  found  degeneration  in  the  olfactory  tubercle,  the 
nucleus  of  the  diagonal  band,  the  prepyriform  cortex 
and  the  periamygdaloid  cortex.  Some  degeneration 
also  occurs  in  the  hypothalamus,  the  mid-line  thalamic 
nuclei,  the  habenular  nucleus  and  the  globus  pallidus. 
No  degeneration  was  .seen  in  the  hippocampal  forma- 
tion or  entorhinal  area,  nor  in  the  lateral  thalamus, 
putamen  or  caudate  nucleus. 


THE    SENSE    OF    SMELL 


545 


ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL  INVESTIGATIONS.  Responses 
can  generally  be  recorded  in  animals  exposed  to 
olfactory  stimuli  from  considerably  wider  areas  than 
those  which  neuroanatomical  studies  have  indi- 
cated as  being  directly  connected  with  the  olfactory 
bulb.  Changes  in  electrical  activity  have  been  reported 
from  the  olfactory  tubercle,  the  septal  region,  the 
prepyriform  and  periamygdaloid  cortex  and  from 
the  hippocampal  formation  (4,  14,  53,  65). 

Direct  electrical  stimulation  of  the  olfactory  bulb 
has  provided  clearer  information  than  the  use  of 
olfactory  stimuli,  since  it  allows  some  assessment  of 
the  temporal  sequence  of  spread  and  mav  permit 
inferences  to  be  drawn  concerning  the  structures  in 
monosynaptic  connection  with  the  olfactory  bulb  (22, 

43.  58,  79)- 

In  the  cat  under  pentobarbital  anesthesia,  the  bi- 
polar record  from  the  prepyriform  cortex  (with  the 
lead  electrode  nearer  the  point  of  stimulation)  shows 
an  initial  fast  negative  spike  with  a  latencv  of  2.0 
msec,  and  presumably  resulting  from  conduction  in 
the  olfactory  tract.  This  is  succeeded  by  a  biphasic 
response  with  an  initial  negativity  peaking  at  6  to  8 
msec.  In  records  from  near  the  caudal  border  of  the 
prepyriform  cortex  the  diphasic  response  appears  as 
a  double  negative  wave.  This  second  peak  is  elimi- 
nated by  repetitive  stimulation,  possibly  from  svn- 
chronization  of  cortical  activity,  in  such  a  way  that 
the  same  elements  which  previously  fired  separately 
to  produce  two  peaks  discharge  in  unison  to  produce 
a  single  larger  response  (43).  This  is  supported  by 
anatomical  studies  in  the  primary  olfactory  cortex 
of  the  mouse  (75)  which  have  disclosed  intracortical 
neuron  chains  possessing  abundant  and  systematically 
distributed  cells  with  short  axis  cylinders  within 
these  chains. 

Similar  records  from  the  surface  of  the  olfactory 
tubercle  in  the  cat  indicate  two  negative  waves  with 
latencies  of  6.0  msec,  and  1 1 .0  msec.  Records  in 
depth  show  a  single  deflection  peaking  at  8.0  msec. 
Surface  records  from  the  pyriform  lobe  usually  show 
two  peaks,  an  initial  diphasic  wave  with  a  latency  of 
8  to  10  msec,  and  a  later  deflection  at  20  to  35  msec. 
Since  this  late  respon.se  is  not  abolished  by  section  of 
the  lateral  olfactory  tract  but  only  by  complete  tran- 
section of  the  prepyriform  cortex,  it  is  suggested  that 
the  late  response  in  pyriform  cortex  depends  on  trans- 
cortical connexions  between  prepyriform  and  more 
caudally  placed  pyriform  cortical  areas  (43). 

Potentials  similar  to  those  in  the  cat  are  obtained 
in    the    monke\     from    stimulation    of    the    olfactorv 


ALLIGATOR 


MACAQUE 


OLF.  B 

FR. PREPYR    AREA 

TEMP  PREPYR 
AREA 


OLF  TUB 
AMYG-     "I 
ENTORHINAL  AREA 
FR    PREPYR    AREA 
OLF     TUB 

TEMP.  PREPYR    AREA 
AMYG 
ENTORHINAL  AREA 


FIG.  II.  The  comparative  extent  of  the  primary  olfactory 
cortex  (shaded  area)  in  alHgator,  rat  and  monkey,  indicating 
the  progressive  reduction  in  the  proportion  of  the  cortical 
mantle  receiving  fibers  directly  from  the  olfactory  bulb  in 
higher  vertebrates.  Abbreviations:  AMYG.,  amygdala;  FR. 
PREPYR. AREA,  frontal  prepyriform  area;  OLF.B.,  olfactory 
bulb;  OLF. TUB.,  olfactory  tubercle;  PREPYR. AREA,  pre- 
pyriform area;  TE.M P. PREPYR. AREA,  temporal  prepyriform 
area,  [.-^fter  .■\llison  (15).] 


bulb  (58).  A  fast  negative  spike  can  be  recorded  in 
the  olfactory  tract,  along  the  lateral  and  medial 
olfactory  striae,  from  the  rostrolateral  portion  of  the 
olfactory  tubercle  and  tip  of  the  hippocampal  gyrus. 
Second  and  third  negative  deflections  appear  after 
7  to  1 1  msec,  and  18  to  45  msec,  in  the  olfactory 
tract,  the  cortex  of  the  posterior  orbital  surface  of 
the  frontal  lobe  just  external  to  the  lateral  olfactory 
stria,  the  rostrolateral  posterior  of  the  olfactory 
tuljercle,  the  limen  insulae  and  the  anterior  end  of 
the  hippocampal  gyrus.  In  subjects  under  very  light 
chloralose  anesthesia,  Kaada  also  noted  responses  in 
the  posterior  part  of  the  hippocainpal  gyrus,  the  hip- 
pocampus and  the  septum  lucidum. 

Centrifugal  influences  may  modify  both  the  resting 
and  induced  electrical  activity  of  the  olfactory  bulb 
(59).  Stimulation  of  the  prepyriform  cortex,  cortical 
amygdaloid  nucleus  and  olfactory  tubercle  is  followed 
by  a  depression  of  electrical  activity  in  the  bulb. 
Similar  effects  follow  high  frequency  stimulation  of 
the  anterior  commissure.  These  influences  are  thought 
to  be  mediated  through  the  granule  cells  of  the  bulb 
and  appear  to  exert  tonic  effects  siinilar  to  those  ob- 
served in  the  modulation  of  spinal  afferent  path- 
ways (51). 


546 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGV    I 


FIG.  12.  Diagrammatic  representation  of  intricate  pathways 
through  which  the  primary  olfactory  cortex  may  be  brought 
into  relation  with  certain  'rhinencephalic'  structures  and  cer- 
tain regions  of  the  diencephalon  and  midbrain.  The  stria 
terminalis  (S.T.^  arises  in  the  amygdala  and  terminates  in 
part  in  the  hypothalamus  (HTP.').  The  amygdala  probably 
also  has  more  direct  hypothalamic  connections  and  is  also 
connected  with  the  septum  (SEPT.)  through  the  diagonal 
band  of  Broca  (D.B.B.}.  The  fornix  bundles  may  convey  ac- 
tivity in  both  directions  between  the  hippocampus  and  the 
septum,  the  anterior  thalamus  (.4)  and  hypothalamus,  which 
terminates  partly  in  the  mammillary  body  (-V/).  The  mam- 
millary  body  establishes  anterior  thalamic  connections  through 
the  mammillothalamic  tract.  The  hippocampus  (HIPP.)  is 
reciprocally  connected  with  the  entorhinal  area  (E.NT.AREA). 
The  tegmental  nuclei  {T.N.')  and  the  periaqueductal  grey 
matter  of  the  midbrain  (M.B.)  may  receive  fibers  from  the 
entorhinal  area  through  the  stria  medullaris  (S.M.)  and 
through  the  periventricular  fiber  systems.  The  tegmental 
nuclei  may  also  be  reciprocally  connected  with  the  mammil- 
lary body  through  the  mammillotegmental  tracts  and  the 
mammillary  peduncle. 


Higher  Order  Olfactory  Connections 

It  is  apparent  from  anatomical  and  physiological 
studies  that  the  primary  olfactory  cortex  is  only  in- 
directly connected  with  many  of  the  cortical  and 
subcortical  regions  included  in  classical  accounts  of 
the  rhinencephalon.  This  is  supported  by  the  long 
latencies  of  responses  recorded  in  the  more  remote 
regions.  In  the  cat  under  pentobarbital  anesthesia, 
whereas  responses  appear  in  the  ventral  part  of  the 
head  of  the  caudate  nucleus  after  3.5  to  12.0  msec, 
and  in  the  hippocampal  gyrus,  subiculum  and  an- 
terior end  of  the  hippocampus  after  7.0  msec,  slower 
responses  are  seen  in  the  ventral  parts  of  the  hippo- 
campus only  after  10  to  33  msec,  and  in  the  caudal 
and  dorsal  regions  of  the  hippocampus  after  1 7  to 
38  msec.  Responses  appear  in  the  stria  medullaris 
after  17  msec,  and  in  the  inammillothalamic  tract 
after  25  to  34  msec  (22). 


Certain  studies  have  emphasized  the  possible  role 
of  such  regions  as  the  septum,  the  hippocampal 
formation  and  the  adjoining  pyriform  cortex  in 
mechanisms  of  alerting  and  emotional  arousal.  Al- 
thotigh  responses  from  olfactory  stimulation  can  be 
recorded  in  these  regions,  they  are  also  accessible  to 
other  sensory  stimuli  including  those  from  tactile, 
\isual  and  auditory  modalities  (46,  50,  57,  65). 
Maclean  ct  al.  (65)  have  recorded  in  the  rabbit 
regular  rhythmic  discharges  at  13  to  20  waves  per 
sec.  in  the  anterior  pyriform  cortex  and  in  the  hippo- 
campal formation  during  respiration  of  smoke-filled 
air.  Similar  responses  were  evoked  by  gustatory  and 
painful  stimulation.  They  concluded  that  the  hippo- 
campus responds  to  olfactory  stimuli  in  a  less  pre- 
dictable manner  and  probably  after  a  longer  latency 
than  the  pyriform  area.  They  did  not  seek  responses 
in  the  posterior  pyriform  area  (entorhinal  area  of 
the  hippocampal  gyrus). 

Although  a  great  body  of  evidence  confirms  the 
role  of  the  anterior  pyriform  or  prepyriform  region 
as  the  major  primary  olfactory  cortical  area,  path- 
ways from  it  to  adjoining  rhinencephalic  structures, 
such  as  the  hippocampus,  may  well  be  circuitous 
(fig.  12).  Thus  neuroanatomical  studies  have  indi- 
cated that  the  amygdaloid  nuclei  project  largely  via 
the  stria  terminalis  i^undles  to  the  hypothalamus 
(3,  42,  44).  The  hypothalamus  in  turn  establishes 
connections  with  midline  and  intralaminar  thalamic 
nuclei  (30,  70).  Here  further  relays  may  pass  via  the 
fornix  to  the  hippocampal  formation  (49,  50),  and 
ultimately  such  activity  may  reach  the  midbrain 
tegmentum  via  the  entorhinal  area  and  the  stria 
medullaris  (2).  Stimulation  of  the  amygdala  (48)  has 
indicated  widespread  subcortical  projections  from 
both  corticomedial  nuclei  (forming  part  of  the 
primary  olfactory  corte.x)  and  from  basolateral  nuclei. 
Short  latency  responses  (presumably  monosynaptic) 
appear  in  o\crlapping  primary  projection  fields 
covering  the  basomedial  part  of  the  telencephalon 
and  adjoining  rostral  pole  of  the  diencephalon.  From 
here  responses  pass  by  short  multisynaptic  relays 
through  secondary  projection  fields  which  include  a 
central  core  of  grey  matter  stretching  from  the  hypo- 
thalamus to  the  midbrain  tegmentum.  Short  latency 
responses  from  the  corticomedial  group  extend 
caudally  into  the  hypothalamus  further  than  those 
following  basolateral  stimulation,  but  no  tegmental 
responses  follow  stimulation  of  the  corticomedial 
nuclei.  This  study  confirms  the  role  of  the  stria  tenni- 
nalis  as  an  important  efferent  pathway  from  the 
amygdala  to  the  hypothalamus. 


THE    SENSE    OF    SMELL 


547 


Behavior  Studies  of  Oljactory  Mechanisms 

Excision  of  the  olfactory  bulb  or  damage  to  the 
anterior  limb  of  the  anterior  commissure  in  the  rat 
leads  to  a  distinct  impairment  of  olfactory  discrimi- 
nation. However,  lesions  involving;  the  septuin,  the 
hippocampus,  the  fimbria,  the  fornix,  the  amygdala 
and  the  pyriform  lobes  are  without  efTect  on  dis- 
crimination in  tests  involving  the  differentiation  of 
wood  shavings  scented  with  oil  of  anise  and  creo- 
sote (82,  83).  Brown  &  Ghiselli  (28)  also  failed  to 
find  any  impairment  of  olfactory  discrimination 
after  a  variety  of  subcortical  lesions.  Experiments  de- 
signed to  test  the  role  in  olfaction  of  pathways  through 
the  anterior  thalamic  nuclei  from  the  hypothalamus 
to  the  cingulate  cortex  have  not  disclosed  impaired 
discrimination  after  total  bilateral  destruction  of  the 
anterior  thalamic  nuclei  and  their  radiations,  with 
additional  involvement  of  the  septum  (63).  In  these 
experiments  removal  of  the  olfactory  bulbs  per- 
manently abolished  the  discrimination  between  the 
odors  of  oil  of  wintergreen  and  of  bread  and  milk, 
indicating  that  the  reaction  was  based  upon  olfactory 
and  not  trigeminal  stimulation.  Cats  are  capable  of 
finding  food  by  olfactory  cues  after  lesions  destroying 
almost  all  the  neocortex  but  leaving  intact  the  an- 
terior pyriform  and  periamygdaloid  cortex  (18). 

Studies  by  Allen  (12,  13)  using  conditioned  re- 
sponses have  provided  a  more  delicate  measure  of 


olfactory  powers  than  simple  discrimination  tests. 
Using  dogs,  Allen  subjected  all  animals  to  four  tests 
which  in\olved  establishment  of  a  conditioned  fore- 
leg response  to  clove  vapor,  ability  to  transfer  this 
refle.x  to  the  opposite  foreleg,  ability  to  establish  an 
absence  of  foreleg  response  to  asafetida  (negative 
conditioned  reflex)  and  differentiation  between  two 
olfactory  conditioned  reflexes  and,  finally,  ability  to 
select  by  smell  w-hen  blindfolded  a  paper  package 
containing  meat  from  three  paper  packets  of  like 
size  and  texture.  Allen  found  that  bilateral  extirpa- 
tion of  the  pyriform-amygdaloid  areas  abolished  the 
negative  conditioned  reflex,  the  animals  raising  the 
foreleg  to  the  odor  of  both  cloves  and  asafetida  after 
cortical  resection.  Additional  ablation  of  the  hippo- 
campal  formation  was  without  effect  on  the  olfactory 
performance,  and  in  no  case  was  the  abilitv  impaired 
in  the  blindfold  test. 

In  view  of  the  close  subjective  relationship  between 
the  senses  of  taste  and  smell,  it  might  be  expected 
that  these  senses  would  activate  the  same  or  adjacent 
cortical  regions.  Quantitative  tests  of  the  monkey's 
preference  for  water  over  a  bitter  quinine  solution 
show,  however,  that  the  insularopercular  cortex, 
rather  than  the  amygdaloid  complex  and  pyriform 
cortex,  is  primarily  concerned  in  taste  (17,  24). 
Human  studies  indicate  that  taste  perception  per- 
sists after  complete  destruction  of  the  olfactorv  nerves 
(29)- 


REFERENCES 

1.  Adey,  W.  R.  Biain  76:  311,   1953.  19. 

2.  Adev,  W.  R.,  N.  C.  R.  Merrillees  and  S.  Sunderland. 

Brain  79:  414,  1956.  20. 

3.  Adey,  W.  R.  and  M.  Meyer.  Brain  75:  358,  1952.  21. 

4.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.  100:  459,  1942.  22. 

5.  Adrian,    E.    D.    Electroencephaloi;.   &    Clin.    .\'furophyuol.  2: 

377.  1950-  23. 

6.  .\drian,  E.  D.  Annee  Psychol.  50:   107,   1950. 

7.*,  Adrian,  E.  D.  Brit.  M.  Bull.  6:  330,  1950.  24. 

8.  Adrian,  E.  D.  iglh  Internal.  Physiol.  Congr.,  Proc:  151,  1953. 

9.  .Adrian,  E.  D.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  29:  5,  1953.  25. 

10.  .'\drian,  E.  D.  Brit.  M.  J.  i :  287,  1954.  26. 

11.  Alexander,   J.    Colloid  Chemistry  (4th    ed.).  New    York;          27. 
Van  Nostrand,  1937.  28. 

12.  Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  128:  754,  1940. 

13.  Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   132:  81,  1941.  29. 

14.  Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  139:  553,  1943.  30. 

15.  Allison,  A.  C.  Biol.  Rev.  28:  195,  1953. 

16.  Allison,  A.   C.   and  R.   T.   T.   Warwick.   Brain  72:    186,  31. 
1949.  3'^- 

17.  Bagshaw,   M.    H.    .and   K.    H.    Pribr.\m.   J.  .Keurophrsiol.          33. 
16:  499,  1953. 

18.  Bard,   P.   and  V.   Mountcastle.   .1.   Res.  .\'erv.  &   .Menl.  34. 
Dis.,  Proc.  27:  362,   1948. 


Beck,   L.   H.,   L.    Kruger   and   P.    Cal.abresi.   .4nn.  j\'ew 
I'ork  Acad.  Sc.  58:  225,  1953. 

Beck,  L.  H.  and  W.  R.  Miles.  Science  106:  511,  1947. 
Beidler,  L.  M.  .inn.  J^'ew  I'ork  Acad.  Sc.  58:  52,  1953. 
Berry,  C.  M.,  VV.  D.  Hagamen  and  J.  Hinsev.  J.  .keuro- 
phrsiol.  15:   139,  1952. 
Bloom,   G.   and   H.    Engstrom.   Exper.    Cell.   Res.    3:   699, 

1 9.52  • 

Blum,  J.  .S.,  K.  L.   Cnow  and  K.  H.  Pribram.  J.  Comp. 

.Veurol.   93:  53,    1950. 

BoDi.AN,  D.  Anat.  Rec.  106:  178,  1950. 

Bourne,  G.  H.  Nature,  London  161:  445,   1948. 

Brodal,  a.  Brain  70:  179,   1947. 

Brown,  C.  W.  and  E.  E.  Ghiselli.  J.  Cnmp.  Psychol.  26: 

'09,  1938. 

Cl.ark,  E.  C.  AND  H.  W.  Dodge.  Neurology  5:  671,  1955. 

Clark,   W.    E.    Le   Gros.    The  Hypothalamus.    Edinburgh: 

Oliver,  1938 

Clark,  VV.  E.  Le  Gros.  .\alure,  London  165:  452,  1950. 

Clark,  \V.  E.  Le  Gros.  Tale  J.  Biol.  &  .Med.  29:  83,  1956. 

Clark,  W.  E.  Le  Gros.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  146: 

^99.  ■957- 

Clark,  VV.   E.   Le  Gros  and  M.   Meyer.   Brain  70:  304, 

1947- 


348 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSI()LO(;V 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


35.  Clark,  W.  E.  Le  Gros  and  R,  T.  T.  Warwick.  J.  Neurol. 
Meurosurg.  &  Psychial.  9:  loi,  1946. 

36.  Deininger,  N.  and  F.  Sullivan.  Ann.  New  York  Acad.  Sc. 
58:  215,  1954. 

37.  Dyson,  G.  M.  Chem.  &  Indus!.  57:  647,  1938. 

38.  El-Bar.\di,  a.  F.  and  G.  H.  Bourne.  Nature,  London  168; 

977.  195'- 

39.  El-Baradi,   a.   F.   and   G.   H.   Bourne.   Science    113;  660, 

■95'- 

40.  Elsberg,  C.  a.  and  I.  Lev^'.  Bull.  .Vcurol.  Insl.  New  Tork. 

4:5.  '9.35- 

41.  Fox,  C.  A.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  72:  i,  1940. 

42.  Fox,  C.  A.  Anat.  Rec.  103:  537,  1949. 

43.  Fox,  C.  A.,  \V.  A.  McKiNLEV  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  7:1,1 944. 

44.  Fox,  C.  .\.  .\ND  ].  T.  ScHMiTZ.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  79:  297, 

'943- 

45.  Gasser,  H.  S.  J.  Gen.  Plivsiol.  39:  473,  1956. 

46.  Gerard,  R.  W.,  VV.  H.  Marshall  and  L.  J.  Saul.  Arch. 
Neurol.  &  Psychial.  36:  675,  1936. 

47.  Gerard,  R.  W.  and  J.  Z.  Young.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London, 
ser.  B.  122:  343,  1937. 

48.  Gloor,    p.    Eleclroencephaloi^.  &    Clin,    .\europhysiol.    7:    223, 

1955- 

49.  Green,   J.  D.  and  \V.   R.  Ade^'.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clm. 
Neurophysiol.   8:    245,    1956. 

50.  Green,  J.   D.   .\nd  .A.   Arduini.  J.  Neurophysiol.    17:  533, 

■954- 

51.  H.'VGBARTH,   K-E.   and   D.I.B.   Kerr.  J.  .Xeuropkysiol.    17: 

295.  ■954- 

52.  Hainer,  R.  M.,  .\.  G.  Emslie  and  N.  .\.  Jacobson.  Ann. 
New  York  Acad.  Sc.  58:  158,  1953. 

53.  Hasama,  B.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  234:  748,  1934. 

54.  Hill,  J.   VV.   and   VV.   H.   Carothers.  J.   Am.   Chem.  Soc. 

55:  5039.  '933- 

55.  Jerome,  E.  A.  Arch.  Psychol.  274;  i,  1942. 

56.  Jones,  F.  N.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  66:  81,  1953. 

57.  Jung,  R.  and  A.   E.   Kornmuller.  Arch.  Psychial.   109:   i, 

■939- 

58.  Kaada,  B.  R.  .icta  physiol.  scandinav.  Suppl.  83,  23:  I,  1951. 

59.  Kerr,  D.  I.  B.  .^nd  K-E.  Hagbarth.  J.  Neurophysiol.   18: 
362,  1955- 


60.  KisTiAKOWSKV,  G.  B.  Science  1 12:  154,  1950. 

61.  Kuehner,  R.  L.  Ann.  New  Tork  Acad.  Sc.  58:  175,  1953. 

62.  Lang,  O.  W.,  L.  Farber  and  F.  Yerman.  Food  Indusl.  17: 

8.  1945- 

63.  Lashlev,  K.  S.  .•\nd  R.  VV.  Sperry.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   139; 

446,  1943- 

64.  Legge,  J.  VV.  Australian  J.  Sc.  15:  159,  1953. 

65.  Maclean,  P.  D.,  N.  H.  Horvvitz  and  F.  Robinson.  Yale  J. 
Biol.  &  Med.  25:  159,   1952. 

66.  Mateson,  J.  F.  Ann.  New  York  Acad.  Sc.  58:  83,  1953. 

67.  Meyer,  M.   ,\nd  A.   C.   Allison.  J.  Neurol.  Neurosurg.   & 
Psychial.    12:  274,    1949. 

68.  Moncrieff,  R.  VV.  Ann.  .\ew  York  Acad.  Sc.  58:  73,  1953. 

69.  Moncrieff,  R.  VV.  J.  Physiol.   130:  543,   1955. 

70.  Morin,  F.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  92:  193,  1950. 

71.  Mozell,  M.  M.  J.  Neurophysiol.  21:  183,   1958. 

72.  Mozell,  M.  M.  and  C.  Pfaffman.  Ann.  .New  York  Acad. 
Sc.  58:  96,   1953. 

73.  Naves,  Y.  R.  Perjumery  Essenl.  Oil  Rec.  42:  147,   1951. 

74.  Ogle,  W.  Med.-Chir.  Tr.  53:  263,  1870. 

75.  O'Leary,  J.  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  67:  i,   1937. 

76.  Ottoson,    D.    Ada  physiol.    scandinav.    Suppl.    122,    35:    i, 
1956. 

77.  Pribram,  K.  H.  and  L.  Kruger.  Ann.  .New  York  Acad.  Sc. 
58:  109,  1953. 

78.  R.-iMON  \'  Cajal,  S.  Studies  on  the  Cerebral  Cortex,  translated 
by  L.  M.  Kraft.  London:  Lloyd-Luke,  1956. 

79.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  Fed.  Proc.  2:  42,  1943. 

80.  Sem-Jacobsen,  C.  VV.,  R.  G.  Bickford,  H.  J.  Dodge  and 
C.  Peterson.  Proc.  Staff  Meet.  Mayo  Clin.  28:  166,  1953. 

81 .  Sumner,  J.  B.  Ann.  .New  York  Acad.  Sc.  58:  68,  1953. 

82.  SwANN,  H.  G.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  59:  175,  1934. 

83.  SvifANN,  H.  G.  Am.  J.  Physiol,  iii:  257,  1935. 

84.  Teudt,  H.  Prometheus  30:  201,  1919. 

85.  Turk,  A.  Ann.  New  York  Acad.  Sc.  58:  193,  1953. 

86.  Walsh,  R.  R.  Fed.  Proc.  12:  150,  1953. 

87.  Wenzel,  B.  M.  Psychol.  Bull.  45:  231,  1948. 

88.  Wenzel,  B.  M.  Science  121  :  802,  1955. 

89.  Young,  C.  W.,  D.  E.  Pletcher  and  N.  Wright.  .Siience 
1 08 :  4 1 1 ,  1 948. 

90.  Zwaardemaker,  H.  .4rch.  neerl.  Physiol.  6:  336,    1922. 


CHAPTER    XXII 


Vestibular   mechanisms 


B.    E.   GERNANDT     I     Department  of  Physiology,  University  of  Gothenburg,  Gothenburg,  Sweden 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Anatomy  of  Labyrinth 
Crista 
Macula 
Innervation  of  Sensory  Cells 
Mode  of  Action  of  Vestibular  Apparatus 
Action  of  Semicircular  Canals 
Adequate  stimulation 
Inadequate  stimulation 
Caloric  stimulation 
Galvanic  stimulation 
Action  of  Otolith  Organs 
Labyrinthine  Pathways  and  Reflexes 
Ascending  Fibers 
Nystagmus 
Cortical  projection 
Descending  Tracts 
Effects  of  Labyrinthectomy 


THE  INNER  EAR  Contains  an  auditory  portion,  the 
cochlea,  and  a  nonauditory  portion  for  maintenance 
of  equilibrium  and  orientation  in  three-dimensional 
space.  The  association  of  two  apparently  very  differ- 
ent functions  in  a  single  organ  may  at  first  seem 
puzzling,  but  the  explanation  for  this  is  found  by 
studying  the  past  history  of  the  ear.  In  this  chapter 
we  are  concerned  only  with  the  nonacoustic  part 
which  we  shall  refer  to  as  the  vestibular  apparatus  or 
the  labyrinth.  This  lodges  the  three  semicircular 
canals  and  two  little  membranous  sacs,  the  utricle 
and  the  saccule.  Their  function  is  to  respond  to  forces 
of  acceleration,  retardation  and  gravitation.  In  lower 
vertebrates,  in  fish  and  even  in  amphibians,  the  sac- 
cule seems  to  play  an  auditory  receptor  role  in  the 
absence  of  the  cochlea.  The  labyrinthine  function  is 
phylogenetically  older  than  that  of  hearing. 

The  labyrinth  is  by  no  means  the  only  sensory  or- 
gan concerned  with  the  control  of  equilibrium.  The 


ability  of  terrestrial  man  and  his  close  relatives  among 
the  vertebrates  to  maintain  equilibrium  and  orienta- 
tion with  respect  to  the  environment  also  depends 
upon  the  stream  of  afTerent  impulses  from  other  re- 
ceptor systems.  These  are  a)  the  eyes  (perception  of 
spatial  relationships),  b)  the  interoceptors  of  the  mu.s- 
cles,  tendons,  joints  and  viscera  and  c)  the  extero- 
ceptors  of  the  skin  (perception  of  position  and  move- 
ment of  the  f^ody  or  changes  in  either  function). 

At  the  beginning  of  the  nineteenth  century  Floin^ens 
(34)  published  the  first  exact  observations  on  the  func- 
tion of  the  semicircular  canals  of  pigeons  and  raliijits. 
He  was  able  to  induce  forced  movements  of  the  head 
and  body  and  involuntary  rhythmical,  conjugate  de- 
viations of  the  eyes  following  injury  to  the  canals. 

Since  then  an  immen.se  amount  of  research  work 
has  been  carried  out.  The  early  part  of  this  period 
was  characterized  by  the  struggle  to  separate  the 
vestibular  apparatus  from  partnership  with  the  coch- 
lea in  the  perception  of  sound  and  to  attribute  to  it  a 
function  quite  unrelated  to  that  of  hearing.  In  1870, 
Goltz  (46)  was  the  first  to  arrive  at  the  conclusion 
that  the  semicircular  canals  were  sense  organs  con- 
cerned with  maintaining  equilibrium. 

The  use  of  cla.ssical  histological  methods  and  the 
observation  of  equilibrium  disturbances  resulting 
from  operative  interference  with  the  internal  ear 
(section  or  extirpation)  have  in  the  past  been  the  two 
principal  sources  of  knowledge  concerning  the  struc- 
ture and  function  of  the  laijyrinth,  but  the  answers 
given  to  various  questions  vary  considerably  in  their 
value.  From  this  it  was  realized  that  knowledge  of 
behavior  mechanisms  in  the  normal  subject  was 
necessary  in  order  to  understand  the  nature  and  sig- 
nificance of  defects  associated  with  peripheral  or  cen- 
tral injuries.  Recording  of  electrical  activity  from 
single  fibers  of  the  peripheral  vestibular  nerve  or  from 
nuclei  within  the  central  nervous  system  of  different 


349 


55° 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


species  has  added  much  to  our  understanding  of  the 
function  of  the  human  labyrinth  in  spite  of  a  number 
of  difficulties  posed  by  the  anatomical  differences  from 
lower  animals.  Experiments  on  various  members  of 
the  animal  kingdom  have  shown  that  some  of  the 
responses  will  vary  greatly  from  one  species  to  another. 
However,  the  use  of  electrophysiological  techniques 
for  a  more  far-reaching  study  of  the  function,  and  the 
refinement  in  recent  years  of  the  ultrastructural  anal- 
ysis made  possible  by  the  electron  microscope  may- 
allow  more  precise  experimental  studies  of  the  corre- 
lation of  function  and  structure.  Some  of  our  modern 
ideas  about  this  correlation  rest  firmly  on  experi- 
mental facts;  others,  in  the  present  incomplete  state 
of  knowledge,  are  mere  speculations.  The  remaining 
ones  ranee  between  these  two  extremes. 


AN.-SiTOMY    OF    LABYRINTH 


lar  canal  makes  an  angle  of  about  55°,  open  in  front, 
and  with  the  posterior  canal,  an  angle  of  about  45°, 
open  posteriorly  (fig.  2). 

The  semicircular  canals  run  from  and  open  into 
the  utricle  by  means  of  five  apertures,  one  being  com- 
mon to  the  superior  and  posterior  canals.  At  one  end 
of  each  canal,  near  its  junction  with  the  utricle,  is  the 
swelling  known  as  the  ampulla.  The  horizontal  and 
superior  canals  have  the  ampulla  forward  and  the 
posterior  canal  has  it  backward. 

The  utricle  is  the  larger  of  the  two  sac-like  struc- 
tures. It  has  an  irregularly  oblong  shape,  slightly 
compressed  transversely.  Its  most  caudal  portion  lies 
posteriorly,  wherefrom  it  slopes  anteriorly  and  up- 
ward (rostrally)  at  an  angle  of  approximately  30°. 
The  utricle  communicates  with  the  utriculosaccular 
duct  and  with  the  semicircular  canals  mentioned 
above. 


For  understanding  the  function  of  the  semicircular 
canals,  the  utricle  and  saccule,  a  clear  concept  of  their 
anatomical  position  and  their  relationship  to  each 
other  in  space  is  paramount. 

The  bony  labyrinth  comprises  a  series  of  ca\ities 
tunneled  in  the  compact  petrous  part  of  the  temporal 
bone.  The  whole  memljranous  labyrinth,  consisting  of 
a  system  of  thin-walled  sacs  and  ducts  with  a  clear 
fluid,  the  endolymph,  is  enclosed  within  the  osseous 
labyrinth,  separated  from  its  wall  by  the  perilymph. 
In  form  it  closely  resembles  the  osseous  labyrinth,  ex- 
cept in  its  middle  portion  (fig.  i). 

The  membranous  semicircular  canals,  three  in 
number  for  each  ear,  are  eccentrically  suspended  by 
fibrous  strands  in  the  osseous  semicircular  canals. 
They  are  smaller  in  diameter  than  the  osseous  canals 
and  fill  only  about  one  fourth  of  the  lumen.  The 
canals  are  named  according  to  their  orientation  in 
space,  the  horizontal  (external,  lateral),  superior 
(frontal)  vertical,  and  posterior  (inferior)  vertical, 
lying  approximately  at  right  angles  to  each  other, 
one  for  each  major  plane  of  the  f)ody.  Considering 
the  two  labyrinths  together  (right  and  left  sides  of 
the  head),  the  two  horizontal  canals  lie  in  the  same 
bodily  plane  and  form  what  may  be  termed  a  synergic 
pair.  The  synergic  partner  of  the  right  superior 
vertical  canal  is  the  left  posterior  \ertical  canal.  The 
left  superior  canal  is  parallel  to  the  right  posterior 
canal.  The  horizontal  canal  is  slightly  inclined  down- 
ward and  backward,  so  that  it  forms  an  angle  of 
about  30°  with  the  horizontal  plane  when  the  head  is 
erect.  The  sagittal  plane  with  the  superior  semicircu- 


PcriostGuTTv 


jcmicirCLilar- 
Carval 


Utr.cle- 
SacTjuIe" 

Oval  window 
Ductus  reumens  " 
Round  window  and 
secondary  tynipanic 
>T\embrane 
Aouoduct  oF  the  "  ' 
cochlea. 


Subdural 
ondoVmphatiC?  sac 
-Dura  nnatrcr 

Aqueduct  of  the 
~    vestibule 
-Endolyrnphatic 
duGtr 

Cochlear 
duct 


■  -^Helicoti-enna 


—  Peri09tCUTT\ 


5cala  Cympani 
Scala  vestibuli 


FIG.  I.   Diagram  of  ppiilymphatic  and  endolymphatic  spaces 
of  the  internal  ear.  [From  Larsell  (57).! 


R.cochl.-sacc 


FIG.  2.  The  innervation  and  structural  relations  of  liuman 
vestibular  apparatus  and  cochlea.  [From  Hardy  (52).] 


VESTIBULAR    MECHANISMS 


551 


The  saccule  is  a  small,  pear-shaped  sac  situated  in 
the  forepart  of  the  vestibule.  It  lies  below  and  medial 
to  the  utricle.  The  long  axis  of  this  sac  is  very  nearly 
vertical;  its  dome-shaped  portion  is  directed  upward 
and  its  bluntly  tapered  portion  downward  and  slightly 
posteriorly.  From  its  posterior  wall,  a  slender  tube, 
the  endolymphatic  duct,  arises  to  extend  through  the 
vestibular  aqueduct  into  the  cranial  cavity;  here  it 
terminates  outside  the  dura  mater  o\cr  the  petrous 
portion  of  the  temporal  bone  in  a  blind  pouch,  the 
endolymphatic  sac.  The  endolymphatic  duct  and  sac 
serve  as  a  drainage  mechanism  for  the  endolymph. 
The  ductus  reuniens,  finally,  is  a  v'ery  slender  duct 
which  connects  the  saccule  with  the  cochlear  duct 
(scala  media)  near  its  basal  end. 


The  macula  is  covered  by  a  mucous  or  gelatinous 
substance  which  contains  aragonite  concretions  (oto- 
liths, otoconia)  of  calcium  carbonate.  The  specific 
gravity  of  the  otoliths,  which  ranges  from  2.93  to  2.95, 
is  thus  greater  than  that  of  the  surrounding  endo- 
hmph.  It  has  been  shown  that  the  otoliths  of  the 
utricular  macula  of  mammals  are  of  two,  or  perhaps 
three,  distinct  grades  of  fineness,  each  kind  being 
situated  in  its  own  particular  area  of  the  receptor  sur- 
face, which  thus  has  a  mosaic  arrangement  (61). 

The  macula  of  the  utricle  is  situated  on  its  anterior 
and  medial  walls,  the  two  portions  being  joined  at  an 
angle  of  140°.  When  the  head  is  in  the  normal  posi- 
tion, the  macula  of  the  utricle  is  in  an  approximately 


Crista 

The  sensory  epithelium  in  the  ampullae  of  the 
semicircular  canals  is  collected  into  transverse  crest- 
like elevations — the  cristae  ampuHares — protruding 
toward  the  lumen  and  firmly  attached  to  their  bony 
foundations  but  free  to  swing  at  the  other  end.  These 
are  the  receptor  organs  of  the  canals.  The  height  of 
the  crista  corresponds  to  about  one  third  the  diameter 
of  the  ampulla.  The  epithelium  is  composed  of  two 
main  types  of  cells,  the  hair  (sensory)  cells  and  the 
nonsensory  supporting  cells.  Recent  electronmicro- 
scopic  studies  have  revealed  two  types  of  hair  cells 
that  differ  distinctly  from  each  other  both  in  structure 
and  innervation  (121).  One  type  of  cells  is  bottle- 
shaped,  the  other  is  more  cylindrical  (fig.  3).  The 
former  is  mainly  localized  to  the  summit  and  the  latter 
to  the  periphery  of  the  cristae.  The  majority  of  them 
have  a  sterocilial  structure,  though  one  process  from 
each  cell  has  a  kinocilium-like  structure.  The  sensory 
hairs,  or  cilia,  project  into  a  gelatinous  mass,  the 
cupula,  and  there  are  found  in  a  large  number  of 
canals  (55,  56;  fig.  4).  The  cupula  may  be  regarded 
as  a  damped  structure  with  a  natural  period,  in  the 
case  of  the  pike  of  about  20  sec.  (106),  and  acts  as  a 
spring-loaded  over-critically  damped  torsion  pendu- 
lum (50).  Its  chemical  structure  is  not  yet  fully 
elucidated,  but  histochemical  investigations  conducted 
in  recent  years  suggest  that  sulphomucopolysaccha- 
rides  are  important  chemical  constituents. 

Macula 

The  receptor  organs  of  the  utricle  and  saccule  are 
called  maculae.  The  sensory  epithelium  exhibits 
again  two  kinds  of  cells,  supporting  and  hair  cells. 


FIG.  3.  The  ultrastructural  architecture  of  the  cells  and 
nerve  endings  of  the  crista  ampullaris  (guinea  pig).  HC  /, 
bottle-shaped  hair  cell;  HC  II,  cylindrical  hair  cell;  SC,  sup- 
porting cell;  St,  sterocilia;  AC,  kinocilia;  .V,  nucleus;  GA,  Golgi 
apparatus;  IM,  intracellular  membrane  system;  VB,  vesicular 
body;  NC,  nerve  calyx;  RM,  reticular  membrane;  M,  mito- 
chondrion; NE,  nerve  endings;  BM,  basement  membrane; 
jV/.V,  myelinated  nerve;  LG,  lipid  granule;  MV,  microvilli. 
[From  Wersall  (121).] 


552 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  4.   Schematic  three-dimensional  diagram  of  one-half  of 
an  ampullar  crista.  [From  Wersall  (121).] 


horizontal  position,  with  the  otoliths  King  on  the 
hair  cells.  The  saccular  maculae  are  situated  ob- 
liquely, forming  an  angle  of  about  30°  with  the  verti- 
cal plane.  Thus,  when  the  head  is  in  the  erect  posi- 
tion, the  otoliths  of  the  saccule  are  placed  laterally  on 
the  hair  cells,  embedded  in  the  substance  that  covers 
them. 


INNERVATION    OF    SENSORY    CELLS 

Impulses  from  the  peripheral  receptors  to  the 
stations  in  the  medulla  are  conducted  by  the  \estibu- 
lar  branch  of  the  eighth  nerve.  These  fibers  make  up 
inore  than  half  of  the  nerve  and  number  about  19,000. 
Most  of  them  are  large  myelinated  fillers  (10  to  15  m) 
but  there  are  also  medium  and  fine  fibers  (i  to  2  /x) 
(28,  59,  86,  90,  91).  In  addition,  a  large  number  of 
unmyelinated  fibers  with  diameters  between  0.3  and 
I  M  have  been  described  (121).  Nerve  fibers  of  vary- 
ing diameter,  as  pointed  out  by  Ramon  y  Cajal  C90) 
and  Lorcnte  de  No  (59),  have  a  characteristic  distri- 
bution in  each  crista.  Thus,  the  large  fibers  innervate 
the  central  region,  those  of  medium  size  are  distrib- 
uted to  the  lateral  regions,  while  the  fine  fibers  go  to 
the  basal  region. 


Electronmicroscopic  examination  of  the  innerva- 
tion of  the  sensory  epithelium  in  the  guinea  pig  by 
Wersall  (121}  revealed  two  different  types  of  nerve 
endings.  The  bottle-shaped  hair  cells  have  a  nerve 
calyx  enclosing  the  greater  part  of  the  cell,  while  the 
nerve  branches  form  loops  around  the  basal  part  of 
the  cylindrical  hair  cells  or  terminate  like  btid-shaped 
nerve  endings  (fig.  3).  Stimulation  of  the  former  type 
will  give  rise  to  impulses  conducted  mainly  in  one 
nerve  ending,  the  nerve  calyx,  and  only  in  one  nerve 
fiber.  The  cylindrical  hair  cell,  however,  is  usually  in- 
nervated by  branches  from  several  dififerent  fibers; 
and  several  hair  cells,  often  at  relatively  great  distance 
from  each  other,  are  innervated  by  the  same  fiber. 
The  difference,  in  principle,  between  the  innervating 
characteristics  of  the  two  types  of  sensory  cells  may 
be  of  physiological  importance  but  no  investigations 
have,  as  yet,  been  able  to  re\eal  clear!)'  the  significance 
of  the  postulated  different  functions  in  the  sensory 
epitheliinu  of  the  cristae  anipullares. 

Recently  PetrofT  (84)  has  published  results  of  ex- 
periments with  sectioning  of  the  eighth  nerve  that 
might  point  to  the  existence  of  thin  efferent  fibers  in 
the  vestibular  nerve.  Such  recurrent  or  feed-back 
connections  of  the  auditory  system  have  previously 
been  described.  Thus  Rasmussen  (92,  93)  has  found 
an  efferent  cochlear  bundle  that  forms  a  plexus  at 
the  margin  of  the  osseous  spiral  lamina  around  the 
afferent  fibers.  Galambos  (35)  was  able  to  surpress 
the  expected  inflow  of  auditory  nerve  activity  to 
normal  acoustic  stimuli  by  electrical  stimulation  of 
these  efferent  fibers.  The  function  of  the  vestibular 
efferent  fibers  has  not  yet  been  studied. 

The  vestibular  nerve  has  six  main  branches  of 
origin :  one  each  from  the  posterior,  superior  and 
lateral  ampullae,  and  the  utricle,  and  two  from  the 
saccule.  Galambos  &  Davis  (36)  have  found  In  histo- 
logical methods  that  the  auditory  nerve  in  the  interval 
from  the  internal  meatus  to  the  medulla  contains 
nerve  cell  bodies  which  probably  belong  to  the  coch- 
lear nucleus  and  are  therefore  second-order  neurons 
in  the  auditory  tract.  There  are  reasons  to  believe 
that  corresponding  second-order  neuron  cell  bodies 
can  be  found  in  the  vestibular  portion  of  the  eighth 
nerve  (38). 


MODE    OF    ACTION    OF    VESTIBULAR    APPARATUS 

Though  there  must  be  an  intimate  coordination 
between  activities  of  receptors  situated  in  the  ampullae 
of  the  semicircular  canals  and  those  in  the  vestibular 


VESTIBULAR    MECHANISMS 


553 


sacs,  the  two  sets  of  end  organs  are,  as  described, 
clearly  different  in  detailed  construction  and  they 
function  in  accordance  with  somewhat  different 
principles.  Interpretation  of  the  particular  functional 
role  of  the  different  sensory  endings  of  the  labyrinth 
has  been  exceedingly  difficult  because  of  the  minute- 
ness of  the  organ  and  the  extreme  inaccessibility  of 
the  structures.  The  recording  of  action  potentials 
from  the  peripheral  nerve  or  from  the  central  nervous 
system  in  response  to  vestibular  stimulation  can,  in 
many  cases,  serve  as  a  revealing  index  of  the  validity 
of  the  older  theories  presented  during  the  last  century. 
In  addition,  this  technique  has  been  of  considerable 
importance  in  furthering  the  study  of  the  mode  of 
action  of  the  labyrinthine  sensory  endings  (i,  6,  27, 
38,58,68,80,94,123). 

Action  oj  Semnucular  Canals 

The  semicircular  canals  respond  to  any  one  of  the 
following  forms  of  adequate  and  inadequate  stimula- 
tion: a)  rotation  (angular  stimulation)  of  the  head  in 
a  vertical,  transverse  or  anteroposterior  axis;  ^)  arti- 
ficial mechanical  stimulation;  c)  caloric,  irrigation  of 
the  ear  with  hot  or  cold  water;  and  </)  galvanic 
stimulation. 

ADEQUATE  STIMULATION.  The  anatomical  fact  that 
three  semicircular  canals  are  arranged  in  planes  ap- 
proximately at  right  angles  to  one  another  corre- 
sponds with  the  conclusion  that  their  function  is 
concerned  with  movements  in  the  three  dimensions. 
It  is  generally  accepted  that  the  cristae  ampullares 
are  receptors  for  the  perception  of  rotatory  move- 
ments. 

The  position  of  the  cupulae  is  influenced  by  an 
increase  or  decrease  of  velocity  of  rotation,  i.e.  by 
positive  or  negative  angular  acceleration,  but  they 
are  proi^ablv  not  influenced  by  linear  acceleration 
(1,53,81,88). 

Several  different  theories  have  been  presented  dur- 
ing the  last  century  to  explain  the  physical  changes 
in  the  canals  resulting  in  stimulation  of  the  receptor 
cells  (the  hydrostatic,  hydrodynamic  and  pressure 
theories).  Some  arc  today  only  of  historical  interest. 
The  literature  for  the  first  quarter  of  the  twentieth 
century  has  been  fully  reviewed  (13,  78,  85,  122)  and, 
in  addition,  a  number  of  reviews  dealing  with  more 
recent  studies  have  appeared  (32,  104,  iio,  119). 
According  to  the  hydrodynamic  theory  of  Mach, 
Breuer  and  Clrum  Brown,  the  only  way  in  which  the 
elastic  cupular  ridge   may  be  swayed,   one  way  or 


another,  is  by  the  flow  of  endolymph.  Any  change  in 
speed  of  rotation  will  cause  a  deflection  of  the  cupula 
and  the  hairs  of  the  sensory  cells  by  a  movement  of 
the  endolymph  with  a  resulting  differential  push  and 
pull  upon  the  hairs.  Owing  to  inertia,  the  endolymph 
of  the  involved  pair  of  canals  lags  behind  the  progress 
of  the  wall  of  its  containing  tube  and  therefore  exe- 
cutes a  movement  opposite  to  the  direction  of  turn- 
ing. The  speed  of  endolymph  movement  in  a  semi- 
circular canal  during  increa.sed  acceleration  and  the 
resulting  deviation  of  the  cupula  have  been  calculated 
(95,  96,  106,  108,  109).  Steinhausen  (107,  108,  109) 
was  able  to  demonstrate  that  the  cupula,  spreading  to 
the  sides  and  reaching  to  the  roof  of  the  ampulla, 
glides  during  its  deflection  in  a  swing-door  fashion 
with  a  minimum  of  endolymph  leakage.  Some  authors 
hesitate  to  accept  the  hydrodynamic  theory  because 
of  the  capillary  nature  of  the  canals  and  the  viscosity 
of  the  endolymph  (48,  73,  78).  According  to  Maier  & 
Lion  (77),  however,  endolymphatic  circulation  is 
possible  in  the  minute  canals. 

The  hydrodynamic  theory  is  strongly  supported  by 
experiments  with  direct  observations  on  the  exposed 
semicircular  canals  in  fish.  Through  the  injection  of 
Chinese  ink  into  the  canals  of  the  pike,  which  are 
relatively  large  and  accessible,  Steinhausen  (107,  108, 
109)  was  able  to  make  visible  the  endolymphatic 
current  with  its  corresponding  deflection  of  the  cupula. 
Dohlman  (24)  introduced  a  drop  of  oil  into  the  canal 
and  the  fish  (cod)  was  rotated  while  the  behavior  of 
the  cupula  was  studied.  As  the  rotation  begins  the 
endolymph  in  the  canal  moves,  as  shown  by  the  shift 
in  the  position  of  the  drop  of  oil,  and  the  cupula 
becomes  bent  over  in  the  direction  of  the  endolymph 
movement  (fig.  5).  By  using  direct  manometric  meas- 
urement he  found  cupular  movement  from  pressure 
changes  equal  to  0.05  ml  of  water  (0.00004  gm)- 

The  most  effective  stimulus  to  each  ampulla  is  rota- 
tion of  the  head  in  the  plane  of  its  canal.  But  angular 
acceleration  about  any  axis  that  lies  obliquely  to  this 
plane  may  also  tend  to  disturb  the  internal  liquid 
(69,  III).  A  more  or  less  combined  stimulation  of  the 
ampullar  cristae  may  be  expected  by  movements  of 
the  head  in  any  one  of  the  intermediate  planes.  The 
utricle  is  shared  by  the  three  canals.  Therefore,  the 
question  arises  whether  this  does  not  cause  an  inter- 
ference between  the  canals.  Indeed,  when  the  fluid 
in  one  canal  is  strongly  affected  by  an  acceleration, 
part  of  it  mav  flow  through  into  another  canal.  The 
other  canals  arc,  however,  a  shunt  with  a  high  re- 
sistance, so  that  the  leakage  is  small  (17,  20);  and, 
for    example,    when    angular    stimulation    produces 


554 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


FIG.  5.  The  ampulla  and  semicircular  canal  in  the  living 
state  before  and  during  angular  acceleration.  The  cupula, 
situated  on  the  top  of  the  crista,  traverses  the  entire  lumen  of 
the  ampulla.  A,  the  cupula  in  its  normal  position;  B,  the  cupula 
during  angular  acceleration.  Note  the  shift  of  position  of  the  oil 
droplet  in  the  endolymph  during  acceleration.  [From  Dohlman 
(24)-] 

endolymph  flow  in  the  horizontal  semicircular  canals 
placed  strictly  in  the  horizontal  plane,  no  flow  is 
thought  to  occur  in  the  vertical  canals  (30,  61). 

Let  us  consider  the  case  of  rotation  to  the  right 
after  adjustment  of  the  head  so  that  the  two  horizontal 
semicircular  canals,  the  synergic  pair,  are  in  the  hori- 
zontal plane.  During  such  a  rotation  the  left  ampulla 
is  '  leading'  its  canal  while  the  right  ampulla  is  '  trail- 
ing' behind  its  canal.  The  endolymph,  owing  to  the 
moment  of  inertia,  will  cause  a  deflection  of  the  cupula 
of  the  right  horizontal  canal  in  the  ampuUopetal  direc- 
tion and  an  ampullofugal  deviation  of  the  cupula  of 
the  left  horizontal  semicircular  canal.  When  the  rota- 
tion is  stopped,  the  two  cupulae  will  be  deflected  in 
the  opposite  direction  owing  to  a  backflow  of  endo- 
lymph. Although  movements  of  the  endolymph  stop 
in  about  3  sec,  the  cupula  seems  to  take  about  25  to 
30  sec.  to  return  to  its  resting  po.sition.  During  this 
time,  the  subject  will  experience  a  sensation  of  rota- 
tion in  the  opposite  direction.  The  stimulus  to  the 
cristae  obviously  arises  from  the  swinging  of  the  cupula 
set  up  by  the  endolymph.  However,  the  cupula,  being 
elastic,  returns  to  its  original  position  if  the  speed  of 
angular  stimulation  becomes  constant.  Therefore,  no 
response  to  movements  of  steady  velocity  occurs  be- 
cause the  endolymph,  subject  to  the  frictional  influ- 
ence of  its  enclosing  walls,  takes  up  the  motion  of  its 
canal  and  stimulation  subsides. 

The  threshold  for  perception  of  angular  accelera- 
tion has  been  studied  by  various  methods  (rotating 
chair,  torsion  swing,  after-sensation  time)  and  is  now 
rather  well-established.  The  torsion  swing  appears  to 
be  the  most  sensitive  method  for  measuring  this 
quantity  (49).  The  minimum  value  for  the  perception 
of  rotation  varies  with  the  indicator  used  and  with 


the  method  of  computation.  Mach  (72)  and  Dodge 
(22)  found  a  threshold  value  of  2 °  per  sec,-  The  prod- 
uct of  the  time  and  the  acceleration  required  to  reach 
the  threshold  of  rotational  .sensation  is  constant.  Thus, 
for  reaching  the  threshold,  the  required  acceleration 
is  the  greater,  the  shorter  the  time  of  its  action.  The 
lowest  values  reported  for  the  human  threshold  are 
0.2°  per  sec-  (i  13)  and  0.5°  per  sec^  (5,  23,  49) 

Some  experimental  results  have  led  to  the  conclu- 
sion that  the  crista  is  a  unidirectional  receptor,  capa- 
ble of  being  stimulated  only  in  one  direction  but 
irresponsive  to  deflection  in  the  opposite  direction 
(16,  70,  71,  78,  109,  123).  Ewald  (29)  demonstrated 
that  an  ampuUopetal  cupular  deviation  in  the  hori- 
zontal semicircular  canals  evokes  a  stronger  reaction 
than  a  corresponding  ampullofugal  deflection.  In  the 
vertical  canals  the  efl'ect  of  ampullofugal  flow  is  more 
marked.  There  is  no  explanation  for  this  functional 
diff'erence,  between  the  horizontal  and  vertical  canals, 
a  diff'erence  emphasized  repeatedly  by  many  authors. 

More  recent  experiments,  however,  speak  in  favor 
of  a  bidirectional  function  of  the  semicircular  canals 
(14,  61).  The  clearest  evidence  comes  from  experi- 
ments with  electrical  recording  of  the  action  poten- 
tials set  up  in  the  primary  receptor  fibers  under  condi- 
tions of  natural  stimulation  (68,  69).  By  dividing  the 
intracranial  portions  of  the  different  nerve  branches 
from  the  labyrinth  into  very  slender  filaments,  it  has 
been  possible  to  obtain  o.scillographic  records  of  the 
action  potentials  occurring  in  response  to  various 
kinds  of  stimuli,  and  under  favorable  conditions  it  is 
possible  to  continue  the  process  of  subdivision  until 
only  one  or  two  sensory  units  are  in  functional  con- 
nection with  the  recording  device.  Ashcroft  &  Hall- 
pike  (6)  and  Ross  (94)  made  the  first  successful 
attempts  to  exploit  this  possibility,  using  frogs. 
Mowrer  (80)  recorded  from  the  vestibular  nerve  of 
the  common  painted  terrapin.  Later  Lowenstein  & 
Sand  (68,  69)  made  similar  recording  from  the  dog- 
fish and  ray  and  Ledoux  (58)  from  the  frog.  They 
demonstrated  a  clear  bidirectional  response  against  a 
background  of  a  resting  discharge  which  is  present 
even  when  the  animal  is  in  a  state  of  absolute  rest; 
their  findings  are  in  complete  agreement  with  the 
assumption  that  the  cristae  are  stimulated  as  a  result 
of  positive  and  negative  angular  acceleration.  In- 
creases or  decreases  in  the  resting  discharge  rate  of 
the  sensory  cells  in  the  crista  are  brought  about  by 
the  deformation  of  their  hair  processes  during  deflec- 
tions of  the  cupula.  In  the  horizontal  canals  excitation 
occurs,  and  an  increased  impulse  discharge  can  be 
recorded  when  the  cupula  is  deflected  in  an  ampul- 


■71; 


VESTIBULAR    MECHANISMS 


555 


lopctal  direction,  the  stimulus  being  ainpullopetal 
inertia  movement  of  endolymph.  With  an  increase  of 
stimulus  strength  a  clear  recruitment  of  sensory  units 
can  be  demonstrated.  The  maximum  frequency  is 
evidently  related  to  the  acceleration,  but  o\\  ing  to  the 
deceleration  which  follows  it  is  impossible  to  say  how 
rapidlv  the  receptors  would  become  adapted  to  the 
stimulus.  For  the  study  of  adaptive  behavior,  a  con- 
stant angular  acceleration  would  have  to  be  applied 
for  a  protracted  period  of  time.  Some  results  suggest 
that  the  receptors  adapt  slowly  (i,  94)  but  Hallpike 
&  Hood  (51)  and  Lowenstein  (64)  came  to  the  con- 
clusion that  the  end  organs  show  considerable  adapta- 
tion under  conditions  of  sustained  cupular  deflection. 

An  ampullofugal  deviation  of  the  cupula  of  the 
horizontal  canal  inhibits  the  spontaneous  impulse 
activity.  This  demonstrates  that  a  single  receptor  can 
signal  rotation  in  either  direction  instead  of  one  direc- 
tion only.  In  the  vertical  canals  the  discharge  of 
impulses  is  increased  by  angular  displacements  in 
which  the  ampulla  is  leading__and  an  ampullofugal 
deviation  of  the  cupula  is  elicited.  An  ampullopetal 
deviation  will  cause  an  inhibition.  On  cessation  (or 
deceleration)  of  the  angular  stimulation,  changes 
which  are  the  reverse  of  the  initial  ones  occur.  If  the 
speed  of  rotation  is  maintained  at  a  constant  level, 
the  impulse  frequency  falls  ofT  until  it  has  reached 
the  spontaneous  rate. 

Adrian  (i)  was  the  first  to  use  a  higher  mammal, 
the  cat,  for  recording  the  discharge  following  varying 
stimulation  of  the  labyrinth.  The  activity  was  recorded 
from  the  vestibular  nuclei.  Generally  speaking,  the 
results  obtained  have  not  shown  any  marked  difTer- 
ence  between  the  vestibular  apparatus  of  the  cat  (i, 
38)  or  rabbit  (27)  and  that  of  the  frog  or  the  fish. 
There  are  gravity  receptors  to  signal  the  posture  and 
linear  acceleration  of  the  head,  and  rotation  receptors 
to  signal  the  turning  movements  (fig.  6).  Some  differ- 
ences are  found,  however,  but  they  are  probably  due 
to  recording  from  second-order  neurons  (38).  Units 
associated  with  the  receptors  of  the  horizontal  semi- 
circular canal  showed  an  increase  in  impulse  fre- 
quency in  response  to  rotation  toward  the  side  of 
recording,  while  rotation  in  the  opposite  direction 
inhibited  the  activity.  Sudden  arrest  of  the  rotatory 
movement  resulted  in  a  reduction  in  impulse  discharge 
rate  after  ipsilateral  and  an  increased  discharge  after 
contralateral  acceleration.  This  type  of  response  is 
interpretable  on  the  basis  of  a  mechanical  tension- 
release  theory  for  the  hair  cells,  excitation  being;  the 
result  of  stress,  inhibition  of  release.  In  addition  to 
this  usual  type  of  response,  there  were  units  which 


showed  an  increased  discharge  in  response  to  rotation 
in  both  directions  (i,  27,  38).  Both  the  ampullopetal 
and  ampullofugal  flow  of  endolymph  had  an  excita- 
tory effect.  A  mechanical  tension-release  theory 
would  seem  to  be  still  more  natural  for  these  units 
than  for  units  of  the  previous  type  (51).  The  hair  cells 
may  be  assumed  to  be  pulled  upon  by  the  movement 
of  endolyinph  and  cupula  in  both  directions.  This 
type  of  response  appears  in  about  12  per  cent  of  units. 
An  inhibitory  effect  of  rotation  in  both  directions  has 
been  noted  also  during  recording  of  the  electrical 
activity  from  second-order  neurons.  This  inhibition 
can  hardly  be  regarded  as  due  to  a  peripheral  mech- 
anism, a  fact  suggesting  a  difference  in  function  be- 
tween higher  mammals  and  simpler  organisms.  An 
inhibition  in  both  directions  of  rotation  should,  how- 
ever, not  provide  greater  difficulties  to  a  tension- 
release  theory  than  inhibition  in  one  direction  only. 
In  both  cases  we  have  to  account  for  the  nature  of 
the  release  by  internal  forces  of  tension  for  which  so 
far  there  is  no  evidence.  Once  impelled,  by  the 
mechanical  theory,  to  add  unidirectional  tensile 
forces  inside  the  receptive  organ  to  account  for  these 
findings,  we  might  as  well  assume  the  existence  of 
structures  pulling  upon  the  hair  cells  in  such  a  well- 
balanced  fashion  that  release  follows  when  the  cupula 
swings  either  way.  Alternatively,  the  mechanical 
theory  should  be  given  up  altogether  in  favor  of  the 
assumption  that  the  impulses  recorded  are  from  cell 
bodies  of  second-order  neurons,  and  that  the  pull  on 
certain  hair  cells  sets  up  inhibition  at  the  first  synapse, 
in  the  manner  of  the  well-known  retinal  inhibition. 
This  alternative  seems  to  be  the  more  probable. 
Another  assumption  is  that  these  neurons  may  have 


60 

t^20 

- 

C) 

l_ > 

V 

"  40 
20 

- 

1^" 

i    ,         ,         ,         , 

^1 

RoulM.n 

FIG.  6.  Diagram  to  illustrate  average  time  course  of  impulse 
discharge  from  a  semicircular  canal  showing  after-discharge  and 
silent  periods  when  acceleration  and  deceleration  are  separated 
by  an  intervzJ  of  steady  rotation.  [From  Adrian  (l)] 


556 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


been  in  synaptic  connection  with  path\va\s  deri\cd 
from  the  opposite  labyrinth  (65). 

iNADEquATE  STIMULATION.  Thc  icsults  obtained  by 
using  various  forms  of  inadequate  stimulation  have 
supported  the  assumption  that  the  flow  of  endolymph 
stimulates  the  cristae.  Thus,  the  old  experiment  of 
Ewald  with  his  '  pneumatic  hammer'  illustrates  how 
the  semicircular  canals  are  stimulated.  He  was  able 
to  stimulate  each  canal  separately  in  a  pigeon  by  in- 
creasing or  decreasing  the  pre.ssure  of  the  endolymph. 
Two  small  holes  were  made  in  an  osseous  semicircular 
canal  near  its  smooth  end.  The  hole  farther  from  the 
ampulla  was  sealed  with  amalgam  so  as  to  block  the 
membranous  canal  completely.  The  pneumatic  ham- 
mer, a  small  metal  cylinder  with  a  moving  piston,  was 
cemented  in  the  hole  between  the  plug  and  ampulla. 
Compression  or  decompression  of  the  endolymph 
caused  an  ampullopetal  and  ampullofugal  endolymph 
flow,  respectively.  An  increase  in  pressure  in  the  hori- 
zontal canals  caused  the  head  and  eyes  to  move 
toward  the  opposite  side;  decompression  caused  a 
weaker  reaction  in  the  reverse  direction.  Compression 
and  decompression  of  the  two  vertical  canals  cause 
similar  movements,  but  the  effect  of  ampullofugal 
flow  is  more  marked. 

CALORIG  STIMULATION.  By  this  form  of  stimulation 
movements  in  the  endolymph  are  produced  (7).  On 
irrigating  the  external  auditory  meatus  with  hot  and 
cold  water,  labyrinthine  reactions  appear  becau.se 
convection  currents  are  provoked  in  the  endolymph 
of  that  semicircular  canal  which  is  placed  in  a  vertical 
position  and  changes  in  the  pressure  on  the  ampulla 
result,  cau.sing  the  cupula  to  bend.  The  direction  of 
the  convection  currents  depends  upon  changes  in  the 
specific  gravity  of  the  endolymph  resulting  from  heat- 
ing or  cooling.  Thus  irrigating  the  car  with  cold 
water  causes  currents  toward  the  ampulla  of  a  vertical 
semicircular  canal;  on  irrigation  with  warm  water 
the  endolymph  ri.ses.  The  effect  of  cold  water  is  there- 
fore the  opposite  of  that  of  hot.  The  caloric  test  used 
in  clinical  otology  and  physiological  experiments  has 
the  advantage  over  rotatory  stimulation  in  that  it 
permits  the  examination  of  one  ear  at  a  time.  If  the 
head  is  held  in  various  po.sitions,  any  one  of  the  three 
semicircular  canals  can  be  stimulated;  however,  the 
posterior  canal,  lying  deep  in  the  bone,  is  influenced 
only  slightly.  Hot  or  cold  water  causes  a  greater 
change  in  the  temperature  of  the  endolymph  in  the 


part  of  the  canal  lying  nearer  to  the  external  meatus 
than  in  the  part  more  deeply  situated.  The  tempera- 
ture change  first  reaches  the  horizontal  canal  (23). 

When  the  head  is  inclined  60°  backward,  the 
horizontal  canals  are  brought  into  a  vertical  position. 
Irrigation  of  the  left  ear  of  a  subject  with  warm  water 
or  the  right  ear  with  cold  water  produces  involun- 
tary, rhythmical  conjugate  deviations  of  the  eyes 
(nystagmus)  to  the  right  and  a  tendency  to  fall  to  the 
same  direction.  The  nystagmus  appears  after  a  short 
latency  and  lasts  for  a  varying  time  according,  inter 
alia,  to  the  temperature  employed  and  the  duration 
of  the  irrigation. 

A  direct  effect  of  thermal  stimulation  upon  the 
peripheral  nerve  endings,  in  addition  to  the  indirect 
effect  based  on  movements  of  the  endolymph,  can  not 
always  be  excluded  (38).  This  is  in  accordance  with 
the  assumptions  made  by  Bartels  (8)  and  Breuer  (11) 
that  cold  water  may  lead  to  a  direct  paralysis  of  the 
nerve  endings  and  by  Spiegel  &  Aronson  (102)  who 
found  that  the  nystagmus  due  to  continued  caloric 
stimulation  was  independent  of  the  position  of  the 
head. 

GALV.-VNIG  STi.MULATiON.  Another  way  to  elicit  reflexes 
from  the  labyrinth  is  by  applying  direct  or  alternating 
currents  to  the  ear.  Galvanic  polarization  produces 
impulse  discharges  similar  to  those  occurring  on  nat- 
ural rotatory  stimulation  (64).  No  movements  of  the 
cupula  will  occur  during  this  form  of  stimulation 
(108).  In  the  employment  of  this  method  of  inade- 
quate stimulation  of  the  cristae  or  the  peripheral 
nerve  fibers  themselves,  an  electrode  is  placed  on  one 
of  the  mastoids,  another  electrode  on  a  distant  part 
of  the  body  (monaural  stimulation)  or  on  the  other 
mastoid  (binaural  stimulation).  In  the  latter  case  all 
six  canals  will  be  stimulated  owing  to  the  current 
spread.  The  galvanic  stimulation  will  give  rise  to  a 
mixture  of  horizontal  and  rotatory  eye  movements. 
When  the  cathode  is  on  the  right  mastoid,  the  nys- 
tagmus is  to  the  right  and  vice  versa.  A  reflex  move- 
ment of  the  head  to  the  left  will  result  if  the  cathode 
of  the  circuit  is  applied  to  the  right  mastoid. 

Action  of  Otolith  Organs 

The  anatomical,  physiological  and  physical  factors 
involved  in  the  stimulation  of  thc  maculae  are  some- 
what different  to  those  influencing  the  semicircular 
canals.  Breuer  (12)  realized  that,  although  the  endo- 
lymph is  not  in  motion  when  the  head  is  at  rest,  we 


nevertheless  have  a  sense  of  position.  He  decided, 
therefore,  that  the  otoliths  within  the  utricle  and 
saccule  must  be  responsible  for  the  static  and  posi- 
tional sense.  The  mechanism  of  stimulation  of  the 
receptors  has  been  controversial.  According  to  the 
theory  of  Breuer,  the  gliding  of  the  otoliths  and  bend- 
ing of  the  hairs  of  the  sensory  cells  caused  by  this 
gliding  during  changes  of  the  position  of  the  head  is 
the  stimulus.  This  theory  has  been  rejected  l)y  later 
workers  (7,  78).  The  effective  stimulus  is  now  thought 
to  be  the  pull  of  gravity.  The  sensory  cells  will  be 
differentially  stimulated  in  different  positions  of  the 
head  since  the  otoliths  will  obey  the  law  of  gravity. 
When  the  stimulation  of  the  utricular  maculae  on 
both  sides  is  equalized,  the  sensation  is  that  of  normal 
position,  with  the  vertex  of  the  head  up  and  its  base 
down.  Any  disturbance  of  this  equilibrium,  as  must 
take  place  in  a  changed  position  of  the  head,  neces- 
sarily exerts  a  different  pull  of  gravity  upon  the  re- 
ceptor structures.  Experiments  have  demonstrated 
that  the  utricle  is  the  source  of  responses  to  gravity, 
centrifugal  force  and  linear  acceleration  (i,  63,  66, 
67,  71,  94).  By  these  various  means  of  stimulation  the 
otoliths  are  made  to  change  their  relative  orientation 
with  respect  to  the  underlying  macular  surface. 
Electrical  responses  recorded  from  the  frog  by  Ross 
(94)  made  it  possible  to  distinguish  between  two  types 
of  gravity  receptors.  One  type  responds  when  the 
head  is  tilted  out  of  the  level  position;  the  other  type 
signals  only  the  return  of  the  previously  tilted  head  to 
level.  Cohen  (15)  describes  four  receptor  types  in  the 
lobster.  Adrian  (i)  recorded  the  potentials  appearing 
in  the  vestibular  nuclei  of  cats  when  the  head  is 
tilted.  In  a  lateral  tilt,  with  the  recording  side  lower- 
most, the  frequency  of  the  discharge  increased  with 
increasing  tilting  (fig.  7).  In  no  case  was  there  an 
increase  in  frequency  when  the  tilt  was  in  the  opposite 
sense,  i.e.  raising  the  side  under  examination  and 
lowering  the  other.  The  frequency  of  the  discharge 
declined  very  slowly  as  an  expression  of  a  slow  adapta- 
tion of  the  receptors.  It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the 
responses  of  different  stimuli  (tilting,  rotation)  were 
not  found  in  the  same  parts  of  the  vestibular  nuclei. 
This  may  well  indicate  some  sort  of  functional  locali- 
zation within  the  nuclei. 

The  utricle  appears  to  be  the  organ  of  major  im- 
portance in  postural  reflexes  and  in  the  differential 
distribution  of  muscular  tone  in  the  various  laby- 
rinthine reflexes. 

The  function  of  the  saccule  is  more  obscure  and 
still  imperfectly  known.  It  can  be  destroyed  on  both 


VESTIBULAR    MECHANISMS  557 


10  20 

Inclinaticm  in  degrees 

FIG.  7.  Response  of  gravity  receptors.  Relation  between  tilt 
of  the  head  and  frequency  of  discharge  in  units  from  several 
animals.  The  degree  of  lateral  tilt  of  the  head  is  shown  in  the 
upper  inset.  The  impulses  were  recorded  from  the  right  side 
while  the  head  was  being  tilted  to  the  right.  [From  Adrian  (i).] 


sides  without  disturbing  labyrinthine  reflexes,  even  in 
the  rabbit,  an  animal  in  which  these  reflexes  are  highly 
developed.  It  has  been  considered  that  the  saccule  is 
not  an  essential  part  of  the  vestibular  mechanism  but 
rather  an  organ  associated  with  the  cochlea  and  de- 
signed for  the  perception  of  vibrational  stimuli  (6,  94, 
123).  Vibrations  acting  upon  the  mass  of  otoliths 
should  thus  transmit  corresponding  oscillations  of 
pressure  to  the  ciliate  cells.  More  recent  experiments 
by  Lowenstein  &  Roberts  (67)  upon  elasmobranchs 
have  presented  evidence  that  the  fibers  conducting 
iinpulses  in  response  to  vibrational  stimuli  are  derived 
from  the  anterior  two  thirds  of  the  saccular  inacula 
(and  the  papilla  basilaris  and  macula  neglecta).  In 
higher  vertebrates  the  saccule  has  probably  lost  its 
auditory  function.  It  is  unlikely  that  the  sound  vibra- 
tions transmitted  from  the  oval  window  to  the  peri- 
lymph are  further  propagated  in  that  part  of  the 
labyrinth  represented  by  otolith  organs  and  the  three 
semicircular  canals.  The  only  exception  may  be  the 
effect  of  very  violent  explosive  sounds.  The  wave  of 
pressure  in  the  endolymph  and  perilymph  set  up  by  a 
sudden,  very  loud  sound  may  be  sufficient  to  stimu- 
late the  receptor  cells  of  the  semicircular  canals,  the 
utricle  and  the  saccule.  The  subjective  sensation  is 
then  one  of  vertigo,  or  of  a  sudden  displacement  in 
.space.  The  reflex  response  to  such  stimulation  is  a 
sudden  movement  of  the  head,  such  as  normally  tends 


558 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


to  compensate  for  an  actual  sudden  change  of  position 
in  space  (112,  115)  The  direction  and  character  of 
the  movement  depend  upon  which  of  the  labyrinthine 
sense  organs  are  most  strongly  stimulated.  The  semi- 
circular canals  can  become  sensitive  to  acoustic 
stimulation  when  they  are  artificialh'  exposed  to  it,  as, 
for  instance,  after  a  fenestration  operation  (12,  45, 
112,  114).  This  does  not  mean,  however,  that  sound 
perception  is  in  the  natural  range  of  functions  of  the 
semicircular  canals. 


L.^BYRINTHINE    P.ATHW.^YS    .AND    REFLE.XES 

The  neural  connections  of  the  vestibular  organ 
consist  of  numerous  chains  of  neurons,  reciprocally 
linked  in  many  ways  and  ha\ing  their  synapses  in 
various  anatomical  nuclei.  All  the  chains  work  in 
intimate  collal^oration  and  the  final  pattern  of  reflex 
responses  is  attributable  largeK'  to  the  highly  complex 
integrating  activity  of  the  center  (62).  The  labyrin- 
thine function  is  automatic,  carried  out  in  a  reflex 
fashion,  in  other  words,  mostly  below  the  level  of 
consciousness.  The  brain  centers  through  which  the 
labyrinths  elicit  the  various  appropriate  muscular 
reactions  of  the  head,  body,  limbs  and  eyes — the 
righting,  the  postural  and  the  ocular  reflexes — repre- 
sent an  intricate  mechanism.  The  nervous  connections 
of  the  vestibular  apparatus  with  the  brain  are,  as  yet, 
imperfectly  known. 

The  impulses  generated  in  response  to  stimulation 
of  the  peripheral  receptors  pass  for  the  most  part  to 
Peiter's  nucleus  (the  lateral  \estibular  nuclejis), 
Schwalbe's  nucleus  (the  niedial  nuclejj.s!).  Bechterew's 
nucleus  (the  supfrior  nucleus^  and  Roller's  nucleus 
(the  descending  oi-  spinal  \  estibulaJL  nuclettsy  In 
these  nuclei  originate,  in  turn,  the  ascending  and 
descending  tracts.  For  all  practical  purposes  these 
four  nuclei  can  be  treated  as  a  single  functioning 
entity.  Some  axons  pass  directly  to  the  cerebellum 
(2,  25). 

The  vestibular  nuclei  on  each  side  of  the  medulla 
are  connected  with  each  other.  This  connection  may 
be  either  direct  (19,  31,  47,  90)  or  indirect  by  way  of 
the  reticular  formation.  Ramon  y  Cajal  (90)  describes 
a  compact  bundle  of  fibers  within  the  vestibular 
nerve  which  passes  directly  from  the  vestibular 
(Scarpa's)  ganglion  across  to  the  opposite  side  of  the 
bulb  without  synaptic  relay  in  the  ipsilateral  vestib- 
ular nuclei.  Because  these  fibers  spread  out  diffusely 
after  crossing  the  mid-line,  he  was  uncertain  whether 


they  terminate  in  the  contralateral  vestibular  nuclei 
or  within  the  contralateral  bulbar  reticular  formation. 

Ascending  Fibers 

Fibers  arising  from  the  medial  and  superior  vestib- 
ular  nuclei  form  the  medial  longitudinal  fasciculus, 
the  fibers  of  which  end  in  the  nuclei  of  the  oculomotor 
nerves  of  the  same  and  opposite  sides.  The  tract  is 
phylogenetically  one  of  the  every  early  ones  to 
appear.  It  is  present  in  cyclostomes  and  is  known  to 
be  an  important  reflex  pathway  in  fish.  Its  position 
and  connections  are  very  constant  throughout  the 
vertebral  series. 

NYST.AGMUS.  Thc  position  of  the  eyes  is  very  markedly 
influenced  by  stimulation  set  up  in  the  labyrinth. 
This  is  of  obvious  importance  since,  as  the  body 
moves,  compensation  must  be  made  b\'  the  eye 
muscles  in  order  that  the  gaze  may  remain  fixed  on 
any  object.  In  birds  and  reptiles  most  of  the  compensa- 
tion is  made  by  the  neck  muscles,  and  a  head  nystag- 
mus appears  during  and  after  angular  stimulation 
(12).  As  the  body  turns  the  eyes  swing  slowly  in  the 
opposite  direction  so  as  to  maintain  their  fixation. 
Having  turned  as  far  as  possible,  they  swing  quickly 
back  in  the  opposite  direction  to  fix  a  new  object  which 
in  turn  they  follow  by  a  slow  deviation.  The  slow  move- 
ment in  one  direction  is  known  as  the  slow  component 
of  the  nystagmus,  and  the  quick  movement  in  the 
opposite  direction  is  known  as  the  quick  component. 
The  reflex  latency  of  the  slow  component  is  50  to  80 
msec.  (21).  The  magnitude  of  the  quick  and  slow 
components  is  the  same  and  by  convention  the  direc- 
tion of  the  nystagmus  is  designated  as  that  of  its  quick 
component.  Thus,  when  the  quick  component  of  a 
nystagmus  is  observed  to  be  in  the  direction  of  the 
subject's  right,  it  is  called  a  nystagmus  to  the  right. 
The  movement  of  the  eyes  in  nystagmus  is  in  either 
the  horizontal,  frontal  or  sagittal  plane.  These  differ- 
ent directions  of  the  nystagmus  can  be  easily  demon- 
strated in  man  by  rotating  him  with  eyes  closed  in  a 
revolving  chair  when  different  pairs  of  canals  are 
brought  into  their  maximal  po.sition  (120).  For 
example,  to  stimulate  the  horizontal  canals  maximally 
the  head  should  be  inclined  forward  about  30°.  Dur- 
ing rotation  the  quick  component  will  be  in  ihe 
direction  of  rotation.  When  the  rotation  is  stopped  a 
postrotatory  nystagmus  will  i)e  ob.served;  its  quick 
component  is  in  the  direction  opposite  to  that  of  the 
rotatorv  movement.  This  is  due  to  thc  retardation  of 


VESTIBULAR    MECHANISMS 


559 


the  endolymph  which  causes  a  deviation  of  the 
cupula,  this  time  in  the  opposite  direction.  The  post- 
rotatory  nystagmus  occurs,  and  lasts  as  long  as  the 
cupula  needs  to  return  to  its  starting  position  through 
its  elastic  recoil. 

Thus  we  have  seen  that  the  impulses  from  the  laby- 
rinth are  able  to  act  on  the  different  ocular  muscles  in 
an  extremely  precise  manner.  However,  the  details  of 
the  reflex  arcs  are  as  yet  obscure.  The  slow  phase  of 
nystagmus  is  initiated  from  the  labyrinth  and  has  its 
center  in  the  vestibular  nuclei  from  which  impulses 
are  propagated,  in  part  at  least,  through  the  inedial 
longitudinal  bundles  to  the  eve  muscles.  The  quick 
component  is  entirely  central.  Its  neural  mechanism 
must  lie  in  the  brain  stem  between  and  including  the 
nuclei  foi'the  third  nerves  and  the_yestibular  nuclei. 
for  nystagmus  occurs  after  tran.sections  of  the  brain 
above  and  below  these  levels  (i8,  60,  61).  It  is  not 
abolished  by  ablation  of  the  cerebellum.  Lorente  de 
No  has  located  the  cejUfiT  for  the  rapid  phase  in  the 
reticular  formation  in  the  region  of  the  abducens 
nucleus.  It  has  also  been  found  that  nystagmus  could 
still  be  produced  after  section  of  both  medial  longi- 
tudinal bundles  (60,  61,  98).  This  finding  is  supported 
by  experiments  upon  monkeys  by  Bender  &  Wein- 
stein  (9).  There  may  be  a  double  pathway  from  the 
vestibular  nuclei  to  the  nuclei  of  the  ocular  nerves — 
through  the  medial  longitudinal  bundle  and  through 
the  reticular  formation. 

CORTICAL  PROJECTION.  It  Was  previouslx'  implied  that 
the  vestibular  apparatus  had  only  subcortical  projec- 
tions. Recently,  however,  it  has  been  well  established 
by  the  work  of  a  number  of  investigators  using  electro- 
physiological methods  that  the  organ  is  represented 
by  a  projection  area  in  the  cerebral  cortex  of  the  cat, 
dog  and  monkey.  Adequate  stimulation  (37,  39,  99, 
1 01) — which  is  not  easily  graded  or  measured,  nor 
brief  enough  for  mapping  out  the  exact  boundary  of 
the  area — does  not,  in  the  light  of  more  recent  work, 
seem  to  be  useful.  The  use  of  brief  electrical  stimula- 
tion of  the  vestibular  nerve,  in  order  to  elicit  a  dis- 
crete evoked  cortical  response,  has  been  of  greater 
value  (2,  54,  79,  117).  The  receiving  area  lies  in  the 
anterior  ectosylvian  gyrus_and_tb£_ptQ§terior  bank  of 
the  anterior  suprasylvian  gyrus.  The  projection  is 
princitjallv  contralateral,  but  stimulation  of  the 
ipsilateral  nerve  activates  a  part  of  the  same  region. 
The  response  to  electrical  stimulation  of  the  peripheral 
nerve  occurs  after  a  latency  which  suggests  that  the 


projection  is  direct  from  the  thalamic  relav  nuclei 
(79)-  "  "'  ^  ' 

The  orderly  features  of  the  vestibular  innervation 
and  the  projection  of  the  vestibular  fibers  in  the 
primary  nuclei  (i,  47,  103)  have  prompted  the  postu- 
lation  that  each  vestibular  receptor  organ  has  its  own 
exclusive  representation  on  the  cerebral  cortex. 
Cortical  respon.ses  to  liminal  electrical  stimulation  of 
three  accessible  vestibular  branches  can  be  recorded 
only  from  a  more  limited  portion  of  the  projection 
area  as  a  whole  (fig.  8).  Stimulation  of  the  nerve  from 
the  utricle  of  the  cat  evoked  responses  from  the  dor.sal 
part  of  the  area.  Below  and  anterior  to  the  latter 
focus,  responses  to  stimulation  of  the  ner\e  from  the 
crista  of  the  horizontal  semicircular  canal  were  re- 
corded, and  above  it  the  cortical  projection  of  the 
nerve  from  the  superior  crista  was  found  (2)  (fig.  9). 

It  has  been  demonstrated  by  neurotomy  that  the 
corti^al__response  to  stimulation  of  the  vestibular 
apparatus  requires  neither  an  intact  cerebellum  nor 
an  intact_medial  longitudinal  fasciculus  (4,  87).  This 
proves  that  there  are  other  ascending  \estibular  path- 
ways conducting  the  impulses.  According  to  Wallen- 
berg (i  16)  a  vestibulocortical  pathway  appears  to  run 
parallel  to  the  acoustic  fibers  (10).  It  should  be  noted 
that  available  results  also  suggest  the  existence  of  a 
corticovestibular  connection,  although  it  has  been 
impossible  to  trace  one  (33,  too). 


B 


FIG.  8.  Site  of  electrical  stimulation  ot  branches  of  the  ves- 
tibular nerve.  Ventrolateral  view  of  the  left  \estibule,  A  before 
and  B  after  rcmo\ing  the  membranous  labyrinth.  Vestibular 
nerve  branches  from  the  ampulla  of  the  superior  canal,  /; 
lateral  canal,  2;  and  utricle,  4.  The  utricle  is  marked,  3,  and 
the  saccule,  j.  In  B  three  silver  wires  (black  lines')  are  placed  as 
stimulating  electrodes  and  are  held  in  place  by  dental  cement 
attaching  them  to  the  cut  edge  of  the  bulla.  [From  Andersson 
&  Gernandt  (2).] 


560 


HANDBf)OK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


fk:.  9.  Responses  of  various  cortical  areas  to  vestibular 
stimulation,  recorded  from  points  ;,  2  and  5  indicated  on  the 
drawing  of  the  brain.  A:i,  A:j  and  A:j  from  the  utricle,  B:i, 
B-j  and  B:^  from  the  superior  ampulla;  C.i,  C.jj  and  C.j  from 
the  lateral  ampulla  before  local  strychninization.  A.j,  A.\f  and 
A:6  from  the  utricle;  B:2,  B:^  and  B:6  from  the  superior 
ampulla;  C:2,  C:^  and  C':6  from  the  lateral  ampulla  after 
strychninization.  Time  in  lo  msec,  intervals.  [From  .^ndersson 
&  Gernandt  (2).] 


Descending  Tracts 

Through  these  connections  vestibular  impulses  are 
conveyed  to  the  primary  motoneurons  of  the  spinal 
cord.  As  far  as  origin  and  course  are  concerned  the 
vestibulospinal  tract  seems  to  be  the  less  complicated 
of  the  descending  pathways.  This  tract,  which  origi- 
nates, at  least  for  the  most  part,  from  the  large '  motor' 
type  cells  of  the  lateral  vestibular  nucleus,  descends 
ventrally  during  its  course  in  the  medulla  into  the 
aTUerior_Jmii£iilus  of  the  s^jne  side  of  the  cord.  As 
stated,  numerous  anatomical  connections  exist  between 
the  vestibular  nuclei  and  the  reticular  formation,  and 
the  fibers  which  constitute  the  reticulospinal  tract 
have  been  traced  from  here  into  the  lateral  andven- 
tral  parts  of  the  spinal  cord  (82,  83).  Descending  fibers 
forming  the  nigdial  longilmjjnal  fasciculus,  being  both 
homolateral  and  contralateral,  are  derived  from  the 
descending_ntcdial  and  lateral  nuclei.  Those  fibers  on 
the  contralateral  side  all  terminate  in  the  cervical 
region,  while  those  on  the  homolateral  side  may  con- 
tinue throughout  the  cord. 


X'estibular  and  proprioceptive  systems  are  both 
known  to  be  active  in  posture  and  locomotion; 
streams  of  impulses  arising  from  receptors  in  each  of 
these  systems  must  converge  to  influence  the  activity 
of  the  final  common  path.  ."Ml  reflexes  which  aim  at 
preserving  the  normal  posture  of  the  body  are  col- 
lectively called  'postural  reflexes'  (26,  74,  75,  89); 
these  are  considered  in  Clhapter  XLI  by  Eldred  in 
this  work.  The  hyperextension  of  the  extremities  of  a 
decerebrate  animal  can  be  modified  by  passively 
changing  the  position  of  the  head.  The  compensatory 
movements  of  all  four  legs  are  elicited  by  the  stimula- 
tion of  the  otolith  organ  and  the  proprioceptors  of  the 
neck  muscles.  These  '  tonic  labyrinthine'  and  '  tonic 
neck  reflexes'  operate  in  the  same  direction  and  con- 
sequently sum  algebraically  when  both  arc  elicited. 
The  tonic  labyrinthine  reflexes  can  be  studied  .sepa- 
rately after  excluding  the  tonic  neck  reflexes  by  section 
of  the  upper  cervical  dorsal  roots  or  by  fixing  the  head 
so  as  to  prevent  any  movements  of  it  in  relation  to  the 
body.  It  is  then  possible  to  move  the  animal  about  in 
different  positions  and  thus  ascertain  the  effects  of  the 
labyrinths  upon  the  distribution  of  tone.  For  example, 
placing  the  animal  on  its  back  with  the  angle  of  the 
snout  approximately  45°  ajjove  the  hori7.ontaJ_jTlane 
cau.ses  the  extensor  tone  to  become  maximal;  it  is 
minimal  when  the  animal  is  in  the  prone  position  with 
the  angle  of  the  mouth  45°  below  the  horizontal  axis. 
When  the  head  is  brought  into  other  positions  by 
rotation  of  the  body  around  its  transverse  or  longi- 
tudinal axis,  intermediate  degrees  of  rigidity  between 
the  two  extremes  result.  These  modifications  in  pos- 
tural tone  disappear  if  the  labyrinths  are  destroyed. 
More  precise  experiments  have  made  it  clear  that  the 
reflexes  are  abolished  by  remosing  the  otoliths  from 
their  maculae. 

The  ability  to  stay  in  an  upright  position  is  a  uni- 
versal property  of  man  and  higher  animals.  Five 
principal  groups  of  reflexes  of  a  somewhat  similar 
type,  responsible  for  the  righting  tendency,  have  been 
separated.  Each  one  of  these  factors  alone  may  bring  a 
more  or  less  normal  upright  position;  but  when  they 
collaborate,  greater  precision  and  promptness  in 
righting  results.  These  responses  can  be  studied  in 
decorticate  animals  in  which  their  reflex  nature  is 
quite  apparent.  One  of  them,  which  is  dependent  on 
the  hib\rinth,  will  be  descrilied  briefly.  In  an  animal 
blindfolded  but  with  the  labyrinths  still  intact  the 
head  tends  to  assume  the  natural  horizontal  position 
irrespective  of  the  position  in  space  of  the  remainder 
of  the  body.  The  reflexes  causing  righting  of  the  head, 
initiated    from    the    otolith    organ,    are    called    the 


VESTIBULAR   MECHANISMS 


561 


'labyrinthine  righting  reflexes'.  The  responding 
muscles  are  those  of  the  neck.  The  tonic  labyrinthine 
and  righting  reflexes  are  static  ones  and  are  not  to  be 
confused  with  the  vestibular  reflexes  which  are  pro- 
voked by  movements  in  space  and  initiated  from  the 
semicircular  canals. 

Since  the  classical  investigations  of  Magnus  and 
Sherrington  upon  the  brain-stem  influences  on  spinal 
motor  activity  were  published,  some  more  recent 
papers  concerning  the  maintenance  and  control  of 
static  and  phasic  postural  activites  have  appeared. 
Magoun  and  coworkers  (76)  have  studied  the  role  of 
the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  with  respect  to 
inhibition  and  facilitation  of  spinal  motor  activity. 
The  importance  of  the  vestibular  nuclei  as  an  excita- 
tory mechanism  for  the  cord  has  also,  in  the  light  of 
recent  experiments,  been  reinvestigated  (43,  44,  97, 
105,  118). 

It  has  been  shown  that  the  brain-stem  reticular 
formation  receives  impulses  relayed  from  somatic  and 
auditory  sensory  structures.  It  is  of  interest  from  this 
point  of  view  to  be  able  to  add  the  vestibular  organ 
to  the  rest.  By  recording  the  impulse  activity  generated 
in  response  to  adequate  vestibular  stimulation  from 
isolated  units  in  the  reticular  formation,  it  has  been 
demonstrated  that  the  formation  is  connected  with 
both  the  homolateral  and  the  contralateral  vestibular 
nuclei  (42).  Thus  the  reticular  formation  forms  an 
internuncial  relay  constituting  a  fundamental  element 
of  the  reflex  arc.  The  bilateral  distribution  of  impulses 
from  both  labyrinths  are  changed  in  the  relay  into 
excitatory  and  inhibitory  impul.ses  which  influence 
the  motoneurons,  i.e.  the  vestibular  responses  are 
organized  for  reciprocal  action  on  flexors  and  exten- 
sors even  when  initiated  from  the  reticular  level  C43). 

Impulses  conducted  in  the  vestibulospinal  tract 
will  encounter  fewer  synapses  on  their  wav  from 
periphery  to  periphery  than  those  in  the  reticulo- 
spinal tract  bv  way  of  the  reticular  formation.  It  is 
therefore  possible  to  record  a  two-peak  response  to  a 
single  vestibular  shock  stimulus  from  a  whole  ventral 
root  because  the  impulse  volleys  are  transmitted  along 
separate  paths  having  different  nuclear  delays  (40). 
The  descending  impulses  occurring  in  response  to 
vestibular  stimulation  will  influence  the  activity  of 
both  alpha  and  gamma  fibers.  The  small  gamma 
eflferents,  however,  are  activated  at  a  lower  strength 
of  stimulation  than  are  the  alpha  fibers  (3). 

In  studying  the  effect  of  the  proprioceptive  im- 
pulses upon  the  efferent  discharge  elicited  by  vestib- 
ular stimulation  and  recorded  from  a  ventral  root,  it 
became    obvious    how    strong    and    dominating    this 


FIG.  10.  Effect  of  foot  joint  stimulation  on  vestibular  root 
response.  In  A  is  sliown  a  control  response  recorded  from 
ventral  root  L7.  In  B  the  response  is  augmented  by  manipula- 
tion of  the  tarsometatarsal  joints  of  the  ipsilateral  hind  foot. 
Time  scale  in  msec.  [From  Gernandt  el  at.  (40).] 


proprioceptive  control  can  be.  The  vestibular  re- 
sponse, however  strong  it  may  be,  will  be  inhibited 
by  a  muscular  contraction  (3).  One  kind  of  peripheral 
stimulation  found  to  facilitate  the  vestibular  re- 
sponse arises  from  manipulation  of  the  joints  of  the 
foot  ipsilateral  to  the  recording  site  (fig.  10).  Rein- 
forcement of  the  vestibular  response  by  afferent 
discharges  arising  from  the  foot  joints  will  contribute 
to  the  increased  stability  and  strength  of  the  corre- 
sponding limb  during  standing,  walking  and  jumping 

C40). 

The  effects  of  vestibular  stimulation  upon  strych- 
nine autorh\thmic  convulsive  activity  of  the  spinal 
cord  has  been  studied  in  decerebrated  cats.  Inhibition 
of  strychnine  tetanus  was  obtained  at  all  levels  of  the 
cord  by  tilting  the  head  or  the  whole  animal  to  the 
side,  backward  or  forward.  The  inhibitory  effect  was 
characterized  by  a  progressive  decrease  in  frequency 
of  the  tetanic  waves  until  a  complete,  but  always 
reversible,  inhibition  occurred  (41). 

EFFECTS    OF    LABVRINTHECTOMV 

As  mentioned  above,  a  distinction  is  made  between 
two  different  functions  of  the  vestibular  apparatus. 
One  is  concerned  with  recording  the  position  of  the 
head  in  space,  the  other  with  reacting  to  any  change  in 
the  rate  of  movements.  The  former  function  is 
mediated  by  the  otolith  organs,  the  latter  by  the 
ampullary  cristae  of  the  semicircular  canals.  The 
observation    of    equilibrium    disturbances    resulting 


S62 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


from  operative  interference  with  the  different  parts 
of  the  labyrinth  has  been  an  important  source  of 
knowledge  concerning  the  function  of  the  two  sets  of 
end  organs.  It  was  Ewald  who  first  drew  attention  to 
a  tonic  action  of  the  labyrinth.  The  operation  of 
double  labyrinthectomy  produces  slackness  of  muscles 
in  various  parts  of  the  body.  This  has  been  \erified 
by  McNally  &  Tait  (70)  who  were  aijle  to  show  that 
denervation  of  the  canals__did  not  interfere  with 
muscular  tone,  whereas  denervation  of  the  ujri£le. 
did.  The  general  effects  of  extirpation  of  the  semicircu- 
lar canals,  so  far  as  disturbances  of  equilibrium  and 
occurrence  of  forced  movements  are  concerned, 
resemble  those  resulting  from  operations  upon  the 
cerebellum. 

If  the  organ  on  one  side  is  destroyed,  an  aljnormal 
asymmetrical  posture  of  the  head  and  trunk  results 
ifrom  the  tjnequal  influence  of  the  laljvrinths  on  the 
tnnf  nf  the  nerk  muscles  of  the  two  sides.  The  result 
is  a  continuously  acting  righting  reflex  which  causes 
the  trunk  to  be  curved  and  makes  the  animal  tend  to 
roll  over  and  over.  Cold  blooded  vertebrates  are  much 
disturbed  by  unilateral  ablation  of  the  labyrinths. 
Extirpation  of  the  labyrinth  in  monkeys  is  followed  by 
nystagmus  with  the  quick  component  towards  the 
normal  side  and  rotation  of  head  and  neck  to  the 
same  side.  Rabbits,  cats  and  dogs  are  rather  less 
disturbed. 

In  man  the  effects  are  less  enduring  than  in  the 
monkey.  A  sudden  ablation  or  a  rapid  destruction  of 
one  labvrinth  causes  a  vertigo.  \'estibular  symptoms, 


such  as  nystagmus,  past  pointing,  tendency  to  fall  and 
vertigo,  are  frequently  accompanied  by  symptoms 
pointing  to  an  involvement  of  the  autonomic  system. 
Nausea  and  vomiting,  lowering  of_arteria!  pressure, 
tachycardia  and  recessive  perspiration  may  occur  in 
the  beginning.  The  intensity  of  the  vertigo  renders 
the  sufferer  unable  to  maintain  erect  posture.  \Vhen 
examined  in  bed,  the  patient  is  poised  in  the  least 
uncomfortable  position  and  resists  any  head  move- 
ment for  fear  that  any  alteration  will  increase  the 
vertigo  and  bring  on  a  spell  of  severe  nausea  and  vom- 
iting. The  face  is  pallid  and  the  skin  is  clammy. 
Diarrhea  mav  alternate  with  the  vomiting.  The 
direction  of  the  horizontal  or  rotatory  nystagimis 
present  is  always  to  the  healthy  side.  The  vertigo 
likewise  is  to  the  healthy  side.  The  distressing  vestib- 
ular symptoms  subside  gradually  and  a  complete 
reco\eyy  from  the  vestibular  disability  usually  occurs 
at  the  end  of  one  or  two  months. 

A  complete  bilateral  loss  of  vestibular  function 
does  not  produce  the  vestibular  syndrome  that  is 
found  following  an  acute  destruction  of  one  labyrinth. 
There  is  no  nystagmus  and  novcrtigo.  A  disturbance 
of  equilibrium  is  always  present  and  the  patient,  when 
deprived  of  the  visual  sense,  is  unable  to_mainiain 
normal  posture  and  locomotjon.  When  submerged  in 
water,  he  is  disoriented  and  is  as  likely  to  swim  down- 
ward as  upward  in  attempting  to  reach  the  surface. 
These  symptoms  are  permanent,  although  partial 
compensation  takes  place. 


REFERENCES 

I.   .\dri.^n,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.   lOi  :  389,  1943. 
Q.   Andersson,    S.    .^nd    B.    E.    Gernandt.    Acta    oin-laryng. 
Suppl.  116:  10,  1954- 

3.  Andersson,   S.    and    B.    E.    Gernandt.    J.    ,\europhysiol. 

19:  5'^4.  1956- 

4.  Aronson,  L.  J.  Nerv.  Ment.  Dts.  78;  250,  1933. 

5.  Arslan,  K.  Rev.  taiyiig.  55:  79,  1934. 

6.  Ashcroft,   D.   W.   and  C.   S.   Hallpike.  J.   Laryngol.  £? 
Otol.  49:  450,  1934. 

7.  Barany,  R.    Physiologie   und  Pathologie   drs  Bogengangappa- 
rales  beim  Menschen.  Vienna:  Deuticke,  1907. 

8.  Bartels,  M.  von  Graefes  Arch.  Ophth.  76:  i,  1910. 

9.  Bender,  M.  B.   and  E.   A.  Weinstein.  A.   M.   A.  Arch. 
Neurol  &  Psychiat.  52:  106,  1944. 

10.  Bohm,  E.  and  B.  E.  Gernandt.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  23: 

320,  195'- 

11.  Breuer,  J.  Arch.  ges.  Phyiiol.  44;  135,  1889. 

12.  Breuer,  J.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  48:  195,  1891. 

13.  Camis,  M.    The  Physiology  oj  the  Vestibular  Apparatus.  Ox- 
ford; Clarendon  Press,  1930. 

14.  Cawthorne,  F.  E.,  G.  Fitzgerald  and  C.  S.  Hallpike. 
Brain  65:  138,  1942. 


15.  Cohen,  M.  J.  J.  Physiol.  130:  9,  1955. 

16.  Crum  Brown,  A.  J.  Anal.  Physiol.  8:  327,  1875. 

17.  DE   Burlet,   H.    M.    and   C.   Versteegh.   Acta  uto-rhiiio- 
laryng.  belg.  Suppl.  13:  i,  1930. 

18.  DE  Kleyn,  a.  .\nd  V.   Schenk.  Acta  oto-rhmo-laryng.  belg. 

•5:439.  •93I- 

19.  DE  ViTO,   R.   v.,  .a.  Brusa  and  ."K.   .Arduini.  J.  Neuro- 

physiol.  19:  241,  1956. 

20.  DE  Vries,  H.  Progress  in  Biophysics  and  Biophysical  Chemistry. 
London:  Pergamon  Press,  1956,  vol.  6. 

21.  Dodge,  R.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  4:  247,  1921. 

22.  Dodge,  R.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  6:  107,  1923. 

23.  Dohlman,  G.  Ada  oto-laryng.  Suppl.  5,  1925. 

24.  Dohlman,  G.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  28;  1371,   1935. 

25.  Dow,  R.  S.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:  543,  1939. 

26.  Dusser  de   Barenne,  J.    G.    Handbook  oJ  General  Expert- 
mental  Psychology.  Worcester  Clark  Univ.  Press,  1934. 

27.  Eckel,   VV.   Arch.    Ohren-  Nasen-   u.   Kehlkopjh.    164:   487, 

1954- 

28.  Engstrom,    H.     and    B.    Rexed.    ^Ischr.    mikroskup.-anal 

Forsch.  47  :  448,  1 940. 


VESTIBULAR   MECHANISMS 


563 


29.  EwALD,  J.  R.  Physiologische  Untersuchungen  iiher  das  Endor- 
gan  des  Nervus  Oclavus.  Wiesbaden;  Bergtnann,  1892. 

30.  Favill,  J.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  13:  479,  1925. 

31.  Ferraro,  a.,  B.  L.  Pacella  and  S.  E.  Barrera.  J. 
Comp.  Neurol.  73;  7,  1940. 

32.  Fischer,  J.  J.  The  Labyrinth.  London :  Grune,  1956. 

33.  Fitzgerald,  G.  and  C.  S.  Hallpike.  Brain  65:  115,  1942. 

34.  Flourens,  p.  Recherches  Experimentales  sur  les  Proprieles  et 
les  Fonctions  du  Sysleme  Nerveux  dans  les  Animaux  Vertebres. 
Paris:  Crevot,  1824. 

35.  Galambos,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.  1 9 :  424,  1 956. 

36.  Galambos,  R.  and  H.  Davis.  Science  108:  513,  1948. 

37.  Gerebtzoff,  M.  a.  Arch,  internal,  physiol.  50:  59,  1940. 

38.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  12:  173,  1949. 

39.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  2 1  :  73,  1 950. 

40.  Gernandt,  B.  E.,  Y.  Katsuki  and  R.  B.  Livingston.  J. 
Neurophysiol.  20:  453,  1957. 

41.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  and  C,   A.  Terzuolo.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

183:  I,  1955- 

42.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  and  C.-.^.  Thulin.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  171  : 
121,  1952. 

43.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  and  C.-A.  Thulin.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  172: 

653.  '953- 

44.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  and  C.-A.  Thulin.  Acta  physiol.  scan- 
dinav. 33:  120,  1955. 

45.  Gernandt,  B.  E.  and  M.  Van  Evck.  .Arch,  internat. 
physiol.  61  :  490,  1953. 

46.  Goltz,  F.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  3:  172,  1870. 

47.  Gray,  L.  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  41  :  319,  1926. 

48.  Grey,  E.  G.  Am.  J.  M.  Sc.  151 :  693,  1916. 

49.  Groen,  J.  J.  AND  L.  B.  W.  Jongkees.  J.  Physiol.  107:  i, 
1948. 

50.  Groen,  J.  J.,  O.  Lowenstein  and  .\.  J.  H.  Vendrik. 
J.  Physiol.  117:  329,  1952. 

51.  Hallpike,  C.  S.  and  J.  D.  Hood.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London, 
ser.  B  141:  542,  1953. 

52.  Hardy,  M.  Anat.  Rec.  59:  403,  1934. 

53.  Jongkees,   L.   B.   and  J.  J.   Groen.  J.   Laryng.  G?   Otol. 

61:  529.  '946- 

54.  Kempinsky,  VV.  H.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  14;  203,  1951. 

55.  Kolmer,  W.  Ergebn.  Physiol.  11:  372,  191 1. 

56.  Kolmer,  W.  In :  Handbuch  der  .Mikroskopischen  Anatomic  des 
Menschen  (£,  Gehbrorgan).  Berlin:  Springer,  1927,  vol.  3, 
pt.  I,  p.  250. 

57.  Larsell,  O.  Anatomy  oj  the  Nervous  System  C2nd  ed.). 
New  York :  Appleton,  1 95 1 . 

58.  Ledoux,  a.  .icta  oto-rhino-laryng.  belg.  3 :  335,   1 949. 

59.  Lorente  de  No,  R.   Trab.  Lab.  Invest.  Biol.  Univ.  Madrid 

24:  53.  1926. 

60.  Lorente  de  No,  R.  Labynnthre/lexe  auf  die  .Augenmuskeln. 
Vienna:  Urban,  1928. 

61.  Lorente  de  No,  R.  Ergebn.  Physiol.  32:  73,  1931. 

62.  Lorente   de   No,   R.   Arch.   .Neurol.  &   Psychiat.    30:   245, 

'933- 

63.  Lowenstein,  O.  Nature,  London  161:  652,  1948. 

64.  Lowenstein,  O.  J.  Physiol.  127:  104,  1955. 

65.  Lowenstein,  O.  Brit.  M.  Bull.  12;  114,  1956. 

66.  Lowenstein,  O.  and  T.  D.  M.  Roberts  J.  Physiol,  no: 

392,  1949- 

67.  Lowenstein,  O.  and  T.  D.  M.  Roberts.  J.  Physiol.  114: 

47'.  '93I- 


68. 

69. 

70. 

71- 
72. 

73- 

74- 
75- 

76. 

77- 
78. 

79- 

80. 
81. 
82. 

83- 
84. 

85- 
86. 

87. 


90. 

91- 

92. 

93- 
94- 
95- 
96. 

97- 


99- 
100. 


103. 

104. 

105. 

106. 
107. 
108, 
109, 
1 10, 


Lowenstein,  O.   and  .\.  Sand.  J.  Exper.  Biol.    13:  416, 

1936. 

Lowenstein,  O.  and  A.  Sand.  J.  Physiol.  99:  89,  1940. 

McNally,  W.  J.  and  J.  Tait.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  75:   140, 

'925- 

McNally,  \V.  J.   and  J.  Tait.   Quart.  J.  E.xper.  Physiol 

23:  147.  1933- 

Mach,    E.    Grundlinien   der   Lehre   von   den   Bewegungsempfin- 

dungen.  Leipzig:  Engelman,  1875. 

Mach,    E.    Beitrage    c;/r   Analyse   der    Empfindungen.   Jena 

Fischer,  1886. 

Magnus,  R.  Kbrperstellung.  Berlin:  Springer,  1924. 

Magnus,   R.   and  A.   de   Kleyn.   Arch.  ges.  Physiol.    145; 

455.  1912. 

Magoun,  H.  \V.  Physiol.  Rev.  30:  459,  1950. 

Maier,  M.  and  H.  Lion.  Arch. ges.  Physiol.  187:  47,  1921 

Maxwe;ll,  S.  S.  Labyrinth  and  Equilibrium.    Philadelphia 

Lippincott,  1923. 

MicKLE,  W.   A.   and  H.   W.   Ades.   .4m.  J.  Physiol.    170: 

682,  1952. 

Mowrer,  O.  H.  Science  81  :  180,   1935. 

Mygind,  S.  H.  Acta  oto-laryng.  Suppl.  70:  i,  1948. 

NiEMER,   W.   T.   and  H.   \V.   Magoun.  J.   Comp.  .Neurol. 

87:  367.  1947- 

Papez,  J.  W.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  41  :  365,  1926. 

Petroff,  a.  Anat.  Rec.  no:  505,  1955. 

Pike,  F.  H.  Physiol.  Rev.  3:  209,  1923. 

Polyak,  S.  ^tschr.  Anat.  84:  704,  1927. 

Price,  J.  B.  and  E.  A.  Spiegel.  A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Otolaryng. 

26:658,  1937. 

Quix,  R.  H.  J.  Laryng.  &  Otol.  40:  425,  1925. 

Rademaker,  G.  G.  J.  Das  Stehen.  Berlin:  Springer,  1931. 

Ram6n  y  Cajal,  S.  Histologic  du  Sysleme  .Nerveux  de  l' Homme 

et  des  Vertebres.  Paris:  Maloine,  1909. 

Rasmussen,  a.  T.  Laryngoscope  50:  67,  1940. 

Rasmussen,  G.  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  99:  61,  1953. 

R.'iSMussEN,  G.  L.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  183:  653,  1955. 

Ross,  D.  A.  J.  Physiol.  86:  117,  1936. 

Schmaltz,  G.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  204:  708,  1924. 

Schmaltz,  G.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  208:  424,  1925. 

Schreiner,  L.  H.,  D.  B.  Lindslev  and  H.  W.  Magoun. 

J.  Neurophysiol.  12:  207,  1949. 

Spiegel,  E.  A.  ^tschr.  Hals-Nasen-Olirenh.  25:  200,  1929. 

Spiegel,  E.  A.  J.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  75:  504,  1932. 

Spiegel,   E.   A.   .4.    .A/.   A.   Arch.  Neurol.  &   Psychiat.    29: 

1084,  1933. 

Spiegel,   E.   A.   A.   .M.   A.   Arch.   Neurol.  &  Psychiat.   31: 

469.  1934- 

Spiegel,  E.  A.  and  L.  Aronson.  A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Otolaryng. 

17:  311.  1933- 

Spiegel,  E.  A.  and  L  Sommer.  Neurology  of  the  Eye,  Ear, 

Nose,  and  Throat.  New  York:  Grune,  1944. 

Spiegel,  E.  A.  and  L  Sommer.  Medical  Physics.  Chicago: 

Yr.  Bk.  Pub.,  1947,  vol.  L 

Sprague,  J.  M.,  L.  H.  Schreiner,  D.  B.  Lindslev  and 

H.  VV.  Magoun.  J.  Neurophysiol.  n  :  501,  1948. 

Steinhausen,  W.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  228:  322,  1931. 

Steinhausen,  W.  ^tschr.  Hals-Nasen-Ohrenh.  29:  21 1,  1931. 

Steinhausen,  W.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  232:  505,  1933. 

Steinhausen,  W.  ^ischr.  Hals-Nasen-Ohrenh.  38:  ig,  1935. 

Tait,  J.  Medicine  of  the  Ear.  Edinburgh:  Nelson,  1948. 


564 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


111.  Tait,   J.    AND    W.   J.    McNallv.    Phil.     Trans.    Roy.    Soc. 
London  224:  241,  1934. 

112.  TuLLlo,  P.  Das  Ohr.  Bcilin-VVien:  Urban,  1929. 

113.  TuMARKiN,  I.  A.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  30:  599,  1937. 

114.  Van  Eyck,  M.  Acta  oto-rhino-laryng.  belg.  43:  303,  1953. 
I  15.   VON  Bekesy,  G.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  236:  59,  1935. 

116.  Wallenberg,  A.  Deutsche  ^tschr.  Nervenh.  117:  677,  1931. 

1 1 7.  Walzl,   E.   M.   AND  V.   Mountcastle.   Am.  J.   Physiol. 
159;  595.  1949- 


1 18.  Ward,  A.  A.,  Jr.  J.  JVeurophysiol.  10:  89,  1947. 

119.  Wendt,   G.   R.   Handbook  oj  Experimental  Psychology.  New 
York:  Wiley,  1951. 

120.  Wendt,   G.   R.   and  R.   Dodge.  J.   Comp.   Psychol.  25:  9, 
1938. 

121.  Wersall,  J.  Acta  olo-laryng.  .Suppl.   126;  i,  1956. 

122.  Wilson,  J.  G.  A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Otolaryng.  i  :  231,  1925. 

123.  Zotterm.\n,  Y.  J.  Physiol.  102;  313,  1943. 


CHAPTER    XXIII 


Excitation  of  auditory  receptors' 


H  A  L  L  O  W  E  L  L    DAVIS  Central  Institute  for  the  Deaf,  St.  Louis,  Missouri 


C:  H  A  P  T  E  R     CONTENTS 

Introduction 

Auditory  Information 

Range  and  Differential  Sensitivity 

Significance  of  Bitemporal  Location  of  Ears 
General  Plan  of  Ear 
Functional  Anatomy  and  Acoustic  Properties  of  Ear 

Middle  Eai-:  Acoustic  Impedance  Matching 

Tympanic  Reflex 

Frequency  Characteristics  of  Ear 

Mechanical  Properties  of  Inner  Ear  Structures 

Traveling  Wave  Pattern  of  Cochlear  Partition 

Fine  Structure  of  Organ  of  Corti 

Innervation  of  Hair  Cells 

Fine  Movements  of  Organ  of  Corti 
Blood  Supply  and  Fluids  of  Inner  Ear 

Blood  Supply 

Fluids 
Electric  Responses  of  Inner  Ear 

Action  Potentials 

Intracellular  Potentials 

Endocochlear  Potential 

Cochlear  Microphonic  and  Summating  Potentials 
Auditory  Nerve  Impulses 

Volleys  and  Latencies 

Single  Fiber  Activity 

Efferent  Inhibitory  Action 
Theory  of  Aural  Action 

Transmission  of  Auditory  Information 


INTRODUCTION 

Auditory  Information 

THE  EARS  ARE  SENSE  ORGANS  specialized  for  excitation 
by  airborne  vibratory  energy.   They  belong  in  the 

'  This  work  was  supported  by  a  contract  between  the  Central 
Institute  for  the  Deaf  and  the  Office  of  Naval  Research.  Re- 
production in  whole  or  in  part  is  permitted  for  any  purpose  of 
the  United  States  Government. 


general  class  of  mechanoreceptors,  together  with  the 
organs  of  touch,  pressure,  stretch  and  equilibrium. 
They  are  exteroceptors;  the  source  of  the  acoustic 
energy  is  in  general  external  to  the  body.  They  serve 
to  transmit  information  concerning  the  character  of 
the  physical  source  as  revealed  by  the  rates  of  vibra- 
tion, the  intensity,  the  epoch  and  the  overall  temporal 
pattern  of  .such  vibrations.  The  ears  also  give  informa- 
tion indirectly  as  to  the  direction  from  which  the 
sound  waves  arrive. 

Range  and  Differential  Sensitivity' 

The  lower  frequenc\-  limit  of  '  hearing'  is  usually 
set  arbitrarily  anywhere  from  20  to  50  cps.  Hearing 
merges  gradually  into  sensations  of  touch,  vibration, 
'flutter',  etc.  The  upper  limit  is  about  20,000  cps  in 
young  ears  but  falls  off  with  age.  Differences  in  fre- 
quency of  less  than  one  per  cent  may  be  recognized. 
The  dynamic  range  is  very  great,  covering  more  than 
12  logarithmic  units  (120  db)  on  the  scale  of  acoustic 
energy  (see  fig.  6)  from  a  lower  limit  close  to  the 
physical  background  noise  of  thermal  energy  (Brown- 
ian  movement)  up  to  limits  set  by  acoustic  injurv  to 
the  sense  organ.  Differential  .sen.sitivity  for  intensity  is 
in  the  order  of  magnitude  of  a  tenth  of  a  logarithmic 
unit,  i.e.  one  db.  Absolute  differences  in  time  of  ar- 
rival of  .sound  wavesat  the  twoearsas  small  as  10  msec, 
sensed  in  terms  of  the  direction  of  the  source,  can  be 
detected  by  practiced  observers. 

One  physiological  problem  of  hearing  is  to  under- 
stand how  the  sense  organ  achieves  such  sensitivity, 
dynamic  range  and  discrimination.  Another  is  the 
means  by  which  it  encodes  in  nerve  iinpulses  the  in- 

^  See  especially  the  papers  of  Stevens  &  Davis  (11)  and  of 
von  Bekesy  &  Rosenblith  (22). 


565 


566 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


formation  necessary  for  the  central  nervous  system  to 
achieve  such  discriminations.  A  third  is  the  mecha- 
nism by  which  the  mechanical  forces  of  acoustic 
energy  excite  the  nerve  impulses  in  the  sense  organ. 

Significance  of  Bitemporal  Localion  oj  Ears 

The  location  of  the  inner  ears  within  the  temporal 
bone  of  the  skull  gives  them  excellent  mechanical 
protection.  Certain  details  of  anatomical  architecture 
seem  to  give  special  acoustic  isolation  from  the  inter- 
nal sounds  of  the  body,  including  the  sound  of  one's 
own  voice  (22).  The  location  at  the  sides  of  the  head 
provides  an  acoustic  baffle  between  the  two  ears  that 
insures  the  differences  in  intensity  of  sound  waves  and 
in  times  of  arrival  that  are  the  basis  of  the  sensing  of 
direction  of  the  incoming  waves.  The  location  in  the 
head  also  allows  the  use  of  scanning  movements  of 
the  whole  head,  which,  in  the  human,  replace  the 
separate  movements  of  large  external  ears.  Acous- 
tically, the  human  auricle  is  an  organ  of  little  sig- 
nificance. 


GENERAL    PLAN    OF    EAR 

The  external  ear  (fig.  i)  includes  the  canal  which 
extends  diagonally  inward  about  27  mm  (in  man)  to 
the  tympanic  membrane.   This  partition,   however, 


belongs  to  the  middle  ear  or  'ear  drum.'  The  middle 
ear  is  air-filled  and  is  periodically  ventilated  for 
equalization  of  air  pressure  by  opening  of  the  audi- 
tory (Eustachian)  tube.  The  latter  connects  with  the 
nasopharynx.  A  chain  of  three  small  bones,  the  ossi- 
cles, in  the  middle  ear  form  a  system  of  mechanical 
levers  that  connect  the  outer  tympanic  membrane 
with  a  smaller  inner  opening,  the  oval  window,  into 
the  inner  ear.  A  second  opening  from  middle  to  inner 
ear,  the  round  window,  is  closed  by  the  flexible 
round-window  membrane.  As  we  shall  see,  the  chief 
acoustic  function  of  the  middle  ear  is  to  provide  an 
impedance  match  between  the  air  of  the  external  ear 
canal  and  the  fluid  that  fills  the  inner  ear  and  thus 
to  deliver  acoustic  energy  efficiently  to  the  inner  ear 
where  the  sensory  cells  are  located. 

The  auditory  portion  of  the  inner  ear  is  a  spirally 
coiled  canal,  called  the  cochlea  because  of  its  snail- 
like shape,  within  the  temporal  bone.  It  is  fluid-filled 
and  it  is  divided  along  nearly  its  entire  length  by  a 
partition.  This  partition  is  actually  a  tube,  the  coch- 
lear duct,  which  contains  the  sense  organ  proper, 
namely  the  organ  of  Corti,  and  its  accessory  structure, 
the  tectorial  membrane. 

The  coiled  tube  that  contains  the  organ  of  Corti  is 
roughly  triangular  in  cross  section  (fig.  2).  One  side  is 
formed  by  the  fibrous,  elastic  basilar  membrane, 
which  extends  from  the  inner  bony  core  of  the  coch- 
lea, the  modiolus,  to  the  spiral  ligament  which  lines 


Auricle: 


Cartilage 


SCMICIRCULAR 
MaLLELUS  CANALS 


VeSTIBULE 

VeSTlBULAR    N 

Facial  n 


Cochlear  m 

Internal 
auditory 
canal 

Cochlea 


Mastoid  tip 


Cross  section 
OF  eustachian  tube 


FIG.  I.  In  this  semidiagrammatic  drawing  of  the  ear,  the  cochlea  has  been  turned  slightly  from 
its  normal  orientation  to  show  its  coils  more  clearly.  The  opening  for  nerves  through  the  bone  to 
the  brain  cavity  of  the  skull  is  quite  diagrammatic.  The  muscles  of  the  middle  car  are  omitted. 
[From  Davis  (2).] 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


567 


SCALA    TYMPANI 

(PERILTMPh) 


FIG.  '2.   Cross  section  of  the  cochlear  partition  of  the  guinea  pig  in  the  lower  part  of  the  second 
turn.  [From  Davis  (5).] 


the  outer  wall  of  the  cochlear  canal.  The  organ  of 
Corti  lies  on  the  basilar  membrane.  The  second,  ex- 
ternal, side  of  the  triangle  is  largely  covered  by  the 
stria  vascularis,  so  called  because  it  is  richly  provided 
with  capillaries.  This  thick  layer  of  specialized  cells 
that  face  into  the  cochlear  duct  is  thought  to  secrete 
the  fluid,  the  endolymph,  that  fills  the  duct.  The 
third  side  of  the  cochlear  duct,  Reissner'.s  membrane, 
is  thin  but  double-layered.  It  extends  from  the  edge 
of  the  stria  vascularis  acro.ss  to  the  modiolus  and 
separates  the  space  within  the  cochlear  duct,  the 
scala  inedia,  from  the  .scala  vestibuli.  The  basilar 
membrane  separates  the  scala  media  from  the  scala 
tympani.  The  scala  vestibuli  and  scala  tympani  are 
filled  with  perilymph,  a  fluid  closely  resembling  cere- 
brospinal fluid. 

The  cochlear  partition,  including  both  the  basilar 
membrane  and  Reissncr's  membrane,  ends  a  little 
short  of  the  apical  end  of  the  cochlear  canal  (fig.  3). 
Here  the  scala  vestibuli  and  the  scala  tympani  join 
through  the  helicotrema  while  the  scala  media  ends 
blindly.  At  the  other  end  of  the  scala  tympani  is  the 
round  window.  The  scala  vestibuli  opens  into  the 
central  chamber  of  the  labyrinth,  the  vestibule,  close 
to  the  oval  window.  The  length  of  the  cochlear  parti- 
tion in  inan,  from  its  origin  between  the  oval  and  the 


round  window  to  the  helicotrema,  is  about  35  mm. 
The  sensory  surface  of  the  cochlea  is  thus  a  long 
narrow  ribbon,  coiled  in  spiral  form,  mounted  on  an 
elastic  membrane  between  two  fluid-filled  channels. 
This  membrane  is  moved  by  the  fluid  which  is  driven 
acoustically  at  the  oval  window  by  the  last  of  the 
ossicles,  the  stapes.  The  cochlear  partition  is  the  me- 
chanical frequency  analyzer  of  the  ear. 

FUNCTIONAL    ANATOMY    AND    ACOUSTIC 
PROPERTIES    OF    EAR 

Onlv  those  anatomical  features  of  the  ear  will  be 
described  that  are  necessary  for  understanding  how 
the  ear  acts  as  an  acoustic  impedance  matching  sys- 
tem, an  acoustic  frequency  analyzer  and  a  inechanical 
stimulator.  Anatomy  and  physiological  acoustics  will 
be  combined. 


Middle  Ear:  Acoustic  Impedance  Malcliing^ 

The  tympanic  membrane  is  a  light  but  fairly  stiff 
cone  with  an  apical  angle  in  man  of  about  135°  and 

^  See  especially  the  papers  of  Stuhlman  (13),  von  Bekesy  & 
Rosenblith  (22)  and  \Ve\er  &  Lawrence  (24). 


568 


HANDBOOK    (JF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Perilymph 
Malleus.      Stapes 


Vestibular 
apparatus 


Scala 
vestibuli 


External  auditory  meatus 


Helicotrema 
Scala  tympani 


membrane 


Eustachian^ 
tube 

FIG.  3.   Schematic  drawing  of  tlic  liuman  car.  [From  von  Bekesy  &  Rosenblith  (22).] 


with  flexible  edges.  It  closes  the  external  canal  di- 
agonally. The  long  process  of  the  first  of  the  ossicles, 
the  malleus,  is  attached  radially  to  the  inner  surface 
of  the  tympanic  membrane  from  the  apex  of  the  cone 
nearly  to  its  upper  edge  (figs  4,  5).  The  similarity  of 
the  tympanic  membrane  to  the  paper  cone  of  a  loud- 
speaker or  microphone  is  obvious  and,  like  the  cone 
of  a  microphone,  it  moves  in  and  out  as  a  whole 
when  driven  by  sound  waves — at  least  up  to  about 
2000  cps.  The  malleus  articulates  with  the  second 
ossicle,  the  incus,  but,  except  perhaps  at  very  high 
intensities,  the  coupling  between  them  is  close  and 
they  move  as  one.  The  malleus  and  incus  are  sus- 
pended by  ligaments  in  such  a  way  that  their  only 
free  movement  is  a  rotation  around  an  axis  that  is 
nearly  tangent  to  the  upper  edge  of  the  tympanic 
membrane.  The  membrane  and  the  two  ossicles  turn 
on  this  axis  as  a  unit.  The  rather  large  heads  of 
malleus  and  incus  serve  as  a  counterweight  for  their 
long  processes  so  that  the  center  of  gravity  of  the 
whole  unit  is  very  close  to  its  center  of  rotation.  The 
system  is  therefore  not  readily  set  in  motion  relative 
to  the  head  when  the  head  itself  vibrates.  This  re- 
duces the  sensitivity  of  the  ear  to  bone-conducted 
vibration. 

The  third  ossicle,  the  stapes,  nearly  closes  the  oval 
window  with  its  "foot-plate,"  but  a  flexible  annular 
ligament  allows  it  to  swing  like  a  door  on  an  axis  that 
is  tangent  to  the  oval  window  at  its  posterior  end.  The 
long  process  of  the  incus  articulates  with  the  head  of 
the  stapes  and  drives  the  latter  in  a  bell-crank  type  of 
motion.  When  the  foot-plate  of  the  stapes  moves,  the 


fluid  of  the  inner  ear  moves  with  it.  Although  the 
inner  ear  is  a  closed  chamber,  movement  is  possible 
because  of  the  yielding  of  the  elastic  round  window 
membrane  (fig.  3).  The  latter  thus  moves  in  and  out 
in  approximately  opposite  phase  to  the  foot-plate  of 
the  stapes. 

The  area  of  the  human  tympanic  membrane  is  50 
to  90  mm^.  The  area  of  the  foot-plate  is  about  3.2 
mm'-.  The  amplitude  of  movement  of  the  center  of 
each  is  about  the  same.  In  other  words,  there  is  very 
little  mechanical  advantage  in  the  lever  system  in 
terms  of  amplitude  of  movement.  The  total  force  at 
the  oval  window  is  about  the  same  as  at  the  tympanic 
membrane,  but  it  is  concentrated  in  a  smaller  area; 
therefore,  the  pressure  exerted  on  the  fluid  is  greater. 
The  overall  system  thereby  matches  the  impedance 
of  the  air  almost  exactly  to  that  of  the  inner  ear.  As  a 
result,  very  little  acoustic  energy  is  reflected  back 
from  the  tympanic  membrane  and  nearly  all  is  de- 
livered to  the  inner  ear. 


Tympiniii  Riflrx 

Two  small  muscles,  tensor  tsmpani  and  stapedius, 
attach  to  the  long  process  of  the  malleus  and  the  neck 
of  the  stapes,  respectively.  Each  tends  to  rock  its  ossi- 
cle into  the  cavity  of  the  middle  ear.  The  muscles  are 
thus  mechanically  antagonistic  but  they  act  syner- 
gistically.  They  are  fast  striated  muscles  and  probably 
not  normally  in  tonic  contraction.  They  do  contract 
reflexly,  with  a  latent  period  of  about  10  msec,  in 
response  to  fairly  strong  sounds.  They  also  contract  in 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITOR\'    RECEPTORS 


569 


.Axis  ligaments, 


Ear  drum 


Pivot 


FIG.  4.  Arrangement  of 
incus  and  malleus  showing 
how  the  mass  is  distributed 
around  the  axis  of  rotation. 
The  maximum  displace- 
ment of  the  drum  occurs 
at  its  lower  edge.  [From 
von  Bekesy  (18),  after 
Barany  (i).] 


'  FIG.  5.  The  human 
tympanic  membrane  turns 
on  an  axis  near  its  upper 
rim.  A  fold  on  the  lower 
rim  permits  movement  of 
the  rigid  eardrum  cone. 
[From  von  Bekesy  (18).] 


response  to  mechanical  stimulation  in  the  car  canal. 
The  refle.x  contraction  is  not  very  well  sustained  The 
contraction  does  not  move  the  tympanic  membrane 
significantly  in  man,  but  the  increased  stiffness  (and 
perhaps  damping  also)  of  the  ossicular  chain  reduces 
the  transmission  of  low-frequency  and  of  very-high- 
frequency  sounds.  The  reflex  seems  to  be  primarily 
protective. 

Frequency  Characteristics  of  Ear 

The  middle  ear  has  a  resonant  frequency  of  vibra- 
tion of  about  1700  cps  but  its  movements  are  quite 


heavily,  although  not  critically,  damped.  The  reso- 
nant frequency  of  the  chain  of  ossicles  is  raised  slightly 
when  the  tympanic  muscles  contract.  The  external 
ear  canal  has  a  resonant  frequency  at  about  4000 
cps,  which  gives  an  increase  of  sensitivity  of  about  10 
db  at  this  frequency.  This  resonance  combines  with 
that  of  the  middle  ear  to  give  an  overall  acoustic 
frequency  response  of  the  ear  that  has  a  broad  maxi- 
mum from  800  to  6000  cps  but  which  falls  off  rather 
rapidly  above  6000  and,  less  rapidly,  below  800  cps. 
The  main  features  of  the  human  threshold  curve  of 
acoustic  sensitivity  are  apparently  determined  very 
largely  by  these  acoustic  properties  (fig.  6). 

Meclianical  Properties  oj  Inner  Ear  Structures^ 

In  the  inner  ear  the  basilar  membrane  widens 
gradually  from  0.04  mm  at  the  stapes  to  0.5  mm  at 
the  helicotrema.  Certain  other  measurements,  such  as 
cross  section  of  the  cochlear  canal  and  relative  sizes 
of  certain  types  of  cell  in  the  organ  of  Corti,  are  also 
graded  from  end  to  end;  but  the  important  variation 
that  allows  the  cochlea  to  act  as  a  mechanical  acoustic 
analyzer  is  in  the  width  of  the  basilar  membrane.  As 
a  result  of  this  variation  the  stiffness  ('volume  elas- 
ticity') of  the  cochlear  partition  varies  by  a  factor  of 
at  least  100  from  one  end  to  the  other. 

The  cochlear  partition  has  significant  stiffness  and 
also  inass.  Contrary  to  earlier  opinions  it  is  not  under 
tension.  When  cut  the  edges  do  not  retract.  The  move- 
ments of  the  partition,  like  those  of  the  middle  ear, 
are  quite  heavily,  but  not  critically,  damped.  Because 
of  the  gradation  in  stiffness  and  mass,  different  parts 
of  the  basilar  membrane  have  different  resonant  fre- 
quencies, but  the  various  parts  cannot  move  as  inde- 
pendent resonators.  The  basilar  membrane  and  the 
organ  of  Corti  on  it  are  continuous  structures.  Their 
elements  are  coupled  to  one  another  elastically  and 
also  by  friction.  The  endolymph  and  the  perilymph 
provide  some  of  the  friction. 

Traveling  U^ave  Pattern  nj  Cochlear  Partition'' 

An  increase  in  pressure  on  the  footplate  of  the 
stapes  caused  by  a  sound  wave  sends  a  wave  of 
acoustic  pressure  up  the  cochlea  with  a  velocity  that 
is  determined  by  the  laws  of  transmission  of  acoustic 

'  See  especially  the  papers  of  von  Bekesy  (20)  and  von 
B6kesy  &  Rosenblith  (22). 

'See  especially  the  papers  of  Tasaki  el  at.  (17),  von  Bekesy 
(20,  21)  and  von  Bekesy  &  Rosenblith  (22). 


570 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


100 


I    I  I  1 1  III 1 — Ill 

__  Bekesy  ' 

""■^  .^"Pricking  in  middle  ear"        10 


T      I     I   II  I  I  ij 


"I 1 — I   I  I  I  1 11 


160 

|-140„ 

E 

120  T 

>. 

T3 

100  8 

O 
O) 

-80  i 

c 

60    3 

« 

40   ■? 

.5? 

20 
-0 


100 
Frequency  m  cps 


10,000 


FIG.  6.  The  threshold  of  audibihty  and  the  threshold  of  feeling.  MAP,  minimum  audible  pressure 
at  the  eardrum.  MAF,  minimum  audible  pressure  in  a  free  sound  field,  measured  at  the  place 
where  the  Hstener's  head  had  been.  [From  Licklider  (g).] 


waves  in  a  tube  with  a  flexible  wall.  This  velocity  is 
less  than  the  velocity  of  sound  in  water  but  it  is  so 
fast  that  the  increase  in  pressure  in  scala  vestibuli 
relative  to  scala  tympani  is  virtually  simultaneous 
throughout  the  length  of  the  cochlea.  The  pressure 
wave  travels  much  faster  than  the  phase  velocity  of 
the  traveling  wave  of  mechanical  movement  that 
occurs  in  response  to  this  difference  in  pressure  be- 
tween the  two  scalae.  An  over-all  net  movement  of 
the  cochlear  partition  towards  scala  tympani  in  re- 
sponse to  this  differential  pressure  occurs  because  the 
round  window  forms  a  flexible  portion  of  the  other- 
wise rigid  walls  of  the  bony  labyrinth.  The  round 
window  membrane  bulges  outward  and  thus  allows 
inward  movement  of  the  stapes.  Inside  the  cochlea, 
the  cochlear  partition  bulges  toward  the  round 
window. 

When  the  movement  is  very  slow,  some  fluid  also 
flows  through  the  helicotrema.  But  all  parts  of  the 
cochlear  partition  do  not  move  with  equal  prompt- 
ness. The  relatively  stiff  portion  in  the  basal  turn 
moves  very  nearly  in  phase  with  the  driving  force, 
but  the  more  flexible  apical  portions,  particularly 
those  with  a  resonant  frequency  lower  than  the  fre- 
quency of  the  acoustic  wave  that  is  driving  the  parti- 
tion, tend  to  lag  behind.  As  the  acoustic  wave  reverses 


its  pressure,  the  portion  that  is  'tuned'  to  lower 
frequencies  tends  to  overshoot  and  continues  to  lag 
behind  the  driving  force  exerted  on  it  by  the  acoustic 
pressure  in  the  fluid.  Thus,  because  of  the  gradation 
of  stiffness,  a  traveling  wave  of  displacement  appears 
on  the  cochlear  partition  (fig.  7).  Furthermore,  be- 
cause of  the  continuity  of  the  partition,  the  stifTer 
portion,  moving  almost  as  a  unit,  drives  the  more 
flexible  portion. 

The  traveling  wave  increases  in  amplitude  as  it 
moves  apically  and  reaches  its  maximum  near  the 
region  where  the  resonant  frequency  of  the  basilar 
membrane  corresponds  to  the  frequency  of  the  driving 
waves  (fig.  8).  The  amplitude  of  movement  falls  off 
rather  rapidly  beyond  this  point;  also  the  phase  lag 
increases  rapidly  as  the  traveling  wave  moves  on 
toward  the  apex.  The  velocity  of  travel  therefore 
diminishes,  and  consequenth  the  wavelength  of  the 
displacement  pattern  becomes  shorter.  A  little  distance 
beyond  the  position  of  maximum  amplitude  there  is 
no  significant  movement  at  all.  In  the  region  of  rapid 
diminution  of  amplitude  the  phase  lag  amounts  to  a 
full  cycle  or  more. 

If  the  driving  frequency  is  increased,  the  position  of 
maximum  amplitude  moves  toward  the  oval  window; 
if  it  is  decreased,  the  maximum  moves  toward  the 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


571 


Flg.7 

A<*  =  ^:200cps 


\ 

\ 

— T 

/ 

"~-- 

— 

^ 

N 

\ 

/ 

/        / 

t'O 

"~~-^^ 

^\.          \ 

/ 

/     / 
/   / 

lo. 

1 ..     1 

.    1 

1         1 

1         1         1 

\ 
1 

[           1           r 

1 

_l               0 

20 


22 


24  26  28 

Distance  from  stapes  in  millimeters 


30 


32 


50  100  200  400  800         1600  2400         5000 

Frequency  in  cps 


FIG.  7.  A  traveling  wave  on  the  cochlear  partition  for  a  200  cps  tone.  The  solid  line  shows  the 
pattern  at  one  instant,  the  line  with  short  dashes  a  quarter  of  a  period  later.  The  envelope  shows 
the  maximal  displacement  at  each  point.    [From  von   Bekesy  (19).] 

FIG.  8.  Resonance  curves  for  si.x  points  on  the  basilar  membrane.  The  solid  curves  represent 
measurements  by  von  Bekesy;  the  dashed  curves,  theoretical  calculations  by  Zwislocki.  [From  von 
Bekesy  (22).] 


apex  (fig.  9).  At  about  lOO  cps  in  man,  it  is  very  close 
to  the  helicotrema.  At  2000  cps  there  is  very  Httle 
movement  beyond  the  mid-point  of  the  cochlear  par- 
tition. The  extreme  basal  end  of  the  partition,  how- 
ever, moves  in  response  to  all  frequencies  within  the 
audible  range. 

The  unsymmetrica!  traveling  wave  pattern  of 
movement,  with  its  rather  flat  maximum  of  amplitude 
and  its  abrupt  apical  reduction  in  activity,  has  been 
shown  to  be  a  necessary  and  predictable  consequence 
of  the  principles  of  acoustic  resonance  in  a  system 
such  as  the  cochlea  with  gradation  of  stiffness,  mass, 
damping  and  coupling  (21,  26).  The  traveling  wave 
pattern  has  been  reproduced  in  appropriate  physical 
models  and  it  has  been  observed  directly  in  the 
cochlea  under  the  microscope  with  stroboscopic  illu- 
mination (20)  and  inferred  from  electrical  recordings 
(17)  (see  fig.  15).  It  allows  the  cochlea  to  act  as  a 
mechanical  frequency  analyzer  because  the  extent  of 
activity  and  position  of  maxima  vary  as  functions  of 
frequency.  It  introduces  additional  features,  such  as 
asymmetry,  progressive  time  and  phase  lag,  and  sig- 
nificant longitudinal  as  well  as  transverse  bending  of 
the  cochlear  partition,  that  contribute  to  the  pattern 
of  neural  excitation  that  results  from  the  movements 
of  the  partition. 


Fine  Structure  of  Organ  of  Corti^ 

The  organ  of  Corti  consists  of  sensory  cells  that  are 
known  as  '  hair  cells'  because  of  their  tufts  of  hair-like 


SOOcps     200  cps       100  cps 


50  Cps 


25      Distance  from       3Q 
stapes  in  millimeters 


n 
35 


*  =  0i d:^:::- 


1                         1 

"^-^....^^^       "—  — *-*.^ 

50  cps 

^■^^^^           ^^                            100  cp? 

\              \ 

N^  200  cps\ 
300  cpN              ^ 

'  See  especially  the  review  by  Davis  (4). 


FIG.  9.  Amplitude  and  phase  angle  of  movements  of  the 
cochlear  partition  for  four  different  frequencies  as  a  function  of 
distance  from  the  stapes.  At  50  cps  the  partition  moves  sub- 
stantially in  phase  throughout.  [From  von  Bekesy  (19).] 


processes  that  extend  into  the  scala  media  and  sup- 
porting cells.  The  tectorial  membrane,  in  which  the 
outer  ends  of  the  hairs  are  imbedded,  is  an  important 
accessory  structure  (fig.  2).  It  is  obviously  the  ana- 
logue of  the  otolithic  membrane  of  the  utricle  and  of 
the  cristae  of  the  semicircular  canals,  sense  organs 
that  are  sensitive  to  mechanical  acceleration. 

The  ends  of  Deiters"  cells  that  face  the  scala  media 
form  a  stiff  but  openwork  plate,  the  reticular  lamina. 
The  hair-bearing  ends  of  the  hair  cells  are  firmly  held 
in  the  openings  of  this  lamina;  their  opposite  ends, 
surrounded  by  the  nerve  endings  of  the  auditory  nerve, 
rest  in  cup-like  supports  that  are  also  part  of  Deiters' 
cells.  Between  their  upper  and  lower  ends  the  external 
hair  cells  hang  free  in  a  fluid-filled  space.  The  so- 
called  '  rods  of  Corti'  form,  with  the  basilar  membrane 


572 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


on  which  they  rest,  a  stiff  triangular  supporting  struc- 
ture for  the  inner  end  of  the  reticular  lamina.  The 
outer  edge  of  the  lamina  rests  on  a  softer  cushion  of 
Hensen's  cells.  The  outer  portion  of  the  basilar  mem- 
brane, between  Hensen's  cells  and  the  spiral  ligament, 
carries  the  lower  cuboida!  cells  of  Claudius. 

The  flask-shaped  hair  cells  forming  a  single  row 
along  the  inner  edge  of  the  reticular  lamina  are  known 
as  internal  or  inner  hair  cells.  There  are  about  3500  of 
them,  each  about  12  |i  in  diameter.  The  smaller  (8  fi) 
cylindrical  external  hair  cells  are  arranged  in  three 
or  four  rows  external  to  the  tunnel  of  Corti.  There  are 
aiiout  20,000  of  them  in  each  ear. 


Innervation  of  Hair  Cells'^ 

The  afferent  neurons  of  the  auditory  nerve  art- 
bipolar  cells.  The  cell  bodies,  about  28,000  in  each 
ear,  are  arranged  in  a  long  spiral  ganglion  parallel  to 
the  organ  of  Corti  but  within  the  bony  modiolus 
Their  axons  pass  inward  to  the  hollow  core  and 
thence,  as  the  cochlear  portion  of  the  eighth  cranial 
nerve,  through  the  internal  auditory  meatus,  to  the 
cochlear  nucleus  of  the  medulla.  The  axon-like  den- 
dritic processes  pass  outward  through  the  sieve-like 
bony  and  fibrous  habenula  perforata  into  the  organ 
of  Corti  (fig.  2).  They  are  myelinated  up  to  the 
habenula  perforata.  Some  of  them,  the  internal  radial 
fibers,  pass  directly  to  the  internal  cells  and  innervate 
one  to  three  cells.  Others  cress  the  tunnel  of  Corti  to 
the  external  hair  cells.  Some  of  these  are  radial  fibers 
with  a  restricted  area  of  distribution  but  most  of  them 
run  apically  or  basally,  or  in  both  directions,  for  as 
much  as  several  millimeters  as  the  external  spiral 
fibers.  Each  fiber  innervates  many  external  hair  cells 
but  not  cver\-  cell  along  its  course,  and  each  cell 
typically  receives  more  than  one  nerve  fiber.  The 
plan  of  innervation  is  illustrated  in  figure  10.  The 
ner\c  endings  around  the  lower  ends  of  the  hair  cells 
appear  under  the  electron-microscope  as  well-devel- 
oped structures  rich  in  mitochondria. 

In  addition  to  the  afferent  fillers,  an  efferent  olivo- 
cochlear bundle  from  the  contralateral  olivary  nu- 
cleus runs  lengthwise  of  the  organ  of  Corti  as  the  intra- 
ganglionic  bundle  within  the  modiolus  and  just 
peripheral  to  the  spiral  ganglion  (fig.  2).  These 
efferent  fibers  distriiiute  to  the  organ  of  Corti  and 
apparently  innervate  the  hair  cells,  particularh'  the 
inner  hair  cells. 

'  See  especially  the  papers  of  Dasis  (4)  and  Wever  (23). 


Fine  Movcmenis  of  Organ  of  Corti 

The  fine  movements  of  the  organ  of  Corti  and  the 
tectorial  membrane  have  been  observed  under  the 
microscope  by  stroboscopic  illumination  and  described 
in  some  detail  by  von  Bekesy  (20).  In  any  one  seg- 
ment the  basilar  membrane,  organ  of  Corti,  tectorial 
membrane,  and  usually  Reissner's  membrane  also, 
move  in  phase  with  one  another.  The  basilar  mem- 
brane is  fibrous  and  elastic,  and  basically  it  deter- 
mines the  traveling  wave  pattern  of  vibration  de- 
scribed above.  The  cells  of  Hensen  form  a  soft  cushion 
supporting  the  stiffer  plate  of  the  reticular  lamina. 
The  tectorial  membrane  is  hinged  like  the  cover  of  a 
book  along  the  edge  of  the  limbus.  It  is  composed  of  a 
system  of  diagonal  fibers  and  also  a  jelly-like  sub- 
stance It  is  a  viscous  elastic  system  that  yields  to  slow 
movements  but  is  quite  resistant  to  quick  movements. 
It  returns  rather  slowly  after  being  displaced. 

Apparently,  as  the  basilar  membrane  bulges  'up- 
ward' or  'downward'  (fig.  11),  the  stiff  reticular 
lamina  tends  to  rock  on  the  support  of  the  rods  of 
Corti  around  an  axis  at  the  attachment  of  the  basilar 
membrane  to  the  bony  modiolus.  The  tectorial  mem- 
brane swings  on  its  attachment  to  the  limbus  The 
result  is  a  shearing  action  between  the  tectorial  mem- 
brane and  the  reticular  lamina  (fig  12).  The  'hairs' 
arise  from  the  cuticular  plates  of  the  hair  cells  which 
are  set  firmly  in  the  reticular  lamina,  and  their  outer 
ends  are  firmly  imbedded  in  the  tectorial  membrane. 
Therefore,  as  the  basilar  membrane  bulges,  the  hairs 
are  bent.  The  force  of  the  movements  of  the  cochlear 
partition  is  rather  efficiently  concentrated  on  this 
shearing  action. 

The  movement  described  above  is  associated  with 
an  approximately  radial  displacement  of  Hensen's 
cells,  as  seen  under  the  microscope,  and  a  correspond- 
ing radial  or  slightly  diagonal  bending  of  the  hairs. 
This  movement  is  characteristic  on  the  basal  side  of 
the  position  of  maximal  amplitude.  On  the  apical 
side,  however,  due  to  the  shorter  wavelength  of  the 
traveling  wave  and  sharper  longitudinal  bending  of 
the  basilar  membrane,  a  longitudinal  mo\ement  pre- 
dominates and  the  hairs  are  presumably  bent  longi- 
tudinally instead  of  radially  (fig.  13) 

The  exact  significance  of  these  different  directions 
of  mo\cment  in  relation  to  the  excitation  of  nerve 
impulses  by  the  hair  cells  is  still  a  matter  of  specula- 
tion, but  the  bending  of  the  hairs  is  the  final  and 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS  573 


FIG.  10.  Diagram  of  the  innervation  of  the  cochlea.  The  hair  cells  are  indicated  only  in  part. 
The  principal  types  of  fibers  and  the  bundles  that  they  form  aie:  /  and  -',  intraganglionic  spiral 
fibers;  sa  and  3a,  internal  spiral  fibers;  4,  external  spiral  fibers;  j  and  6,  radial  fibers.  (Based  on 
observations  of  Retzius,  Solovcov  and  Lorente  de  No.)  Not  shown  are  the  relatively  scarce  un- 
branched  external  radial  fibers  (Held).  Type  I  is  the  continuation  of  the  efferent  olivocochlear 
bundle.  [From  Wever  (23).] 


FIG.  II.   Movement  of  the  cochlear  partition,  based  on  descriptions  by  von  Bekesy.  Explanation 
in  text.  [From  Davis  (3).] 


critical  mechanical  event  that  has  been  recognized  in 
the  mechanism  of  stimulation.  At  this  point  the  signif- 
icant events  apparently  become  electrical,  for  this 
bending  of  the  hairs  seems  to  release  energy  in  the 


form  of  biolectric  potentials  and  these  potentials  are 
in  all  probability  the  important  intermediate  step  in 
the  mechanism  of  excitation  of  the  auditory  nerve 
fibers. 


574  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOm'    1 


RETICULAR 


TECTORIAL 


FIG.  12.  Movement  of  the  organ  of  Corti  and  the  tectorial  membrane,  based  on  descriptions  by 
von  Bekesy.  The  shearing  action  between  two  stifT  structures,  the  tectorial  membrane  and  the 
reticular  lamina,  bends  the  hairs  of  the  hair  cells.  [From  Davis  (s)-] 


helicotrema 


stapes 


Hensen  s 
cells 


longitudinal 
vibrations 


radial 


FIG.  13.  The  distribution  of  radial  and  longitudinal  vibra- 
tion along  the  organ  of  Corti  for  stimulation  with  a  tone,  seen 
through  Reissner's  membrane.  [From  von  Bekesy  (20).] 


FLUIDS  AND  BLOOD  SUPPLY  OF  INNER  EAR* 

Blood  Supply 

The  cochlea  is  supplied  by  the  cochlear  artery 
which  enters  the  modiolus  through  the  internal 
auditory  meatus.  The  spiral  ganglion  is  richly  sup- 
plied with  capillaries,  and  arterioles  arch  across  the 
roof  of  the  .scala  vestibuli  to  the  spiral  ligament.  The 
stria  vascularis,  facing  the  scala  media,  is,  as  its  name 
suggests,  a  veritable  maze  of  small  blood  vessels  with 
many  anastomoses.  The  limbus  is  fairly  well  supplied 
with  capillaries  and  a  small  arteriole  often  runs  length- 
wise on  the  tympanic  surface  of  the  basilar  membrane. 

'  See  especially  the  papers  of  Davis  (4)  and  of  Smith  et  al. 
(.0). 


The  blood  flow  of  the  inner  ear  reflects,  as  would  be 
expected,  major  alterations  in  systemic  circulation, 
but  it  does  not  seem  to  be  significantly  aff^ected  by 
stimulation  of  the  cervical  sympathetic  nerve. 

Fluids 

The  perilymph,  which  fills  the  scala  vestibuli  and 
the  scala  tympani,  is  chemically  almost  identical  with 
cerebrospinal  fluid,  and  in  fact  the  perilymphatic 
space  is  anatomically  continuous  with  the  subarach- 
noid space  through  the  cochlear  aqueduct.  Essen- 
tially the  same  fluid  also  permeates  the  modiolus,  the 
substance  of  the  spiral  ligament,  and  the  tunnel  of 
Corti  and  other  spaces  within  the  organ  of  Corti.  The 
basilar  membrane  seems  to  be  readily  permeable  to 
ions,  and,  in  contrast  to  Reissner's  membrane,  offers 
little  resistance  to  electrical  current  flow.  The  sensory 
cells  are  probably  nourished  from  the  scala  tympani, 
not  from  the  scala  media. 

The  endolymph,  which  fills  the  scala  media,  differs 
sharply  from  perilymph  in  its  ionic  content. 
Unlike  all  other  extracellular  body  fluids,  it  is  high 
in  potassium  and  low  in  sodium.  It  more  nearly 
resembles  intracellular  fluid  in  this  respect.  Typical 
analyses  of  endolymph,  perilymph  and  cerebrospinal 
fluid  are  given  in  table  i.  The  endolymph  has  some- 
times been  described  as 'viscous,'  but  this  is  probably 
true  onlv  for  certain  fish  and  perhaps  other  lower 
forms. 

The  endolymph  is  probably  secreted  h\  the  stria 
va.scularis.  Whether  it  is  also  reabsorbed  wholly  or 
onlv  in  part  by  the  same  structure  is  a  matter  of  de- 
bate. The  saccus  endolymphaticus,  an  intradural 
extension  of  the  endolymphatic  system  of  the  mem- 
branous labyrinth,  may  participate  in  secretion,  in 
reabsorption  or  in  both. 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITcmV    RECEPTORS 


0/0 


TABLE  I .  Composition  of  Spinal  Fluid, 
Perilymph  and  Endolymph 


Spinal  fluid 

Perilymph 

Endolymph 

Potassium 

(mEq/1.) 

4.2±o.5 

4.8±o.4 

i44-4±40 

Sodium  (mEq/1.) 

1 52 .  o±  1 . 8 

150. 3±2  .  I 

I5.8±i.6 

Chloride  CmEq/1.)  ^ 

122. 4±i  .0 

12  1  .5±i  .2 

107.  i±i  .4 

Protein  (mg  %) ... 

21     ±2 

50    ±5 

15     ±2 

Results  (means  and  standard  errors  of  means)  of  analyses 
of  spinal  fluid,  perilymph  and  endolymph,  made  by  Smith 
et  al.  (10).  The  endolymph  was  collected  from  the  utricle, 
but  cochlear  endolymph  gave  similar  although  less  reliable 
results. 


ELECTRIC  RESPONSES  OF  INNER  EAR' 

Action  Potentials 

The  final  output  of  the  inner  ear  is  ner\e  impulses 
in  the  auditory  nerve.  If  these  impulses  are  well 
enough  synchronized  into  definite  groups  or  volleys, 
as  in  responses  to  clicks  or  to  successive  sound  waves 
of  a  low-frequency  tone,  the  corresponding  action 
potential  waves  can  easily  be  recorded.  They  appear 
clearly  when  one  electrode  is  on  the  round  window 
and  the  other  on  the  neck,  but  special  placements 
are  needed  to  record  the  action  potentials  without 
contamination  by  the  other  electric  responses  of  the 
cochlea.  With  the  u.sual  electrode  placements,  the 
potentials  are  recorded  as  the  impulses  pass  through 
the  modiolus  and  just  before  they  enter  the  internal 
auditory  meatus. 

The  action  potentials  represent  the  familiar  all-or- 
none 'spike'  responses  of  nerve  fibers.  They  show  defi- 
nite thresholds  and  are  followed  by  refractory 
periods.  One  consequence  of  the  refractory  period  is 
the  phenomenon  of  'masking.'  The  synchronized 
action  potentials  in  response  to  a  click  or  tone  of 
moderate  intensity  are  much  reduced  if  a  moderate 
random  noise  is  presented  at  the  same  time.  The 
noise  stimulates  the  nerve  fibers  at  random  and  the 
refractory  periods  prevent  the  usual  synchronized 
responses  of  many  fibers. 

Other  details  of  the  nerve  response  in  relation  to 
parameters  of  the  stimulus  will  be  given  below.  The 
present  point  is  that  nerve  action  potentials  are  one  of 

'  See  especially  the  papers  of  Davis  (3,  4)  and  of  Tasaki  el 

al.  (15,  16,  17) . 


the  electric  responses  of  the  ear  and  that  they  are  in 
all  ways  similar  to  'axon  spikes'  elsewhere. 

Intracellular  Potentials 

Nearly  all  cells  show  a  negative  intracellular  poten- 
tial. Explorations  of  the  cochlea  and  the  auditory 
nerve  with  very  fine  microelectrodes  reveal  these  intra- 
cellular potentials,  ranging  from  —60  or  even  —80 
mv  relative  to  the  potential  of  the  perilymph  in  large 
cells  such  as  Hensen's  or  Claudius'  down  to  —  20  or 
so  in  the  cells  of  Reissner's  membrane.  The  exact 
value  seems  to  be  a  function  of  the  amount  of  injury 
caused  by  the  microelectrode,  the  greater  the  injury 
the  lower  the  value.  The  hair  cells,  like  the  nerve 
fibers  and  supporting  cells,  are  electrically  negative 
internally. 

Endocochlear  Potential 

The  interior  of  the  scala  media,  the  endolymph,  is 
electrically  positive  relative  to  the  perilymph  in  the 
scala  vestibuli  and  the  scala  tympani,  and  to  the  spiral 
ligament  and  extracochlear  tissues  in  general.  This 
potential  is  -|-8o  mv  (fig.  14).  It  is  encountered 
abruptly  at  the  point  where  the  exploring  electrode 
enters  the  endolymphatic  space,  although  a  relatively 
large  (15  ju)  electrode  pushed  through  the  stria  vascu- 
laris usually  reaches  this  potential  level  in  a  series  of 
two  or  more  steps.  The  change  of  potential  in  going 
from  the  interior  of  a  hair  cell  through  its  cuticular 
layer  into  the  scala  media  is  from  —  70  to  -f-Bo,  or 
about  150  mv. 

The  endocochlear  potential,  formerly  known  as  the 
endolymphatic  potential,  is  so  designated  because  it 
seems  to  be  practically  confined  to  the  endolymphatic 
space  of  the  cochlea.  The  corresponding  potential 
within  the  utricle  is  not  more  than  -I-5  mv.  The  endo- 
utricular  potential  is  hardly  more  than  the  difference 
in  potential  found  in  the  perilymph  between  the 
helicotrema  and  the  basal  end  of  the  scala  vestibuli 
or  scala  tympani.  The  latter  potential  gradient  may 
well  be  due  to  unequal  leakage  through  Reissner's 
membrane  or  other  parts  of  the  endolymphatic  wall, 
but  in  any  case  it  implies  a  considerable  continuing 
current  flow,  dependent  presumably  on  continuing 
metabolic  activity. 

The  endocochlear  potential  is  in  fact  closely  de- 
pendent on  an  adequate  oxygen  supply  It  falls,  re- 
versibly,  to  a  very  low  level  at  the  stage  of  asphyxia 
that  is  reached  in  extreme  Cheyne-Stokes  respiration 


576 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Reissner's 
membrane 


CELLS  OF  THE 
ENDOLYMPHATIC 
WALL 


ENDOLYMPHATIC 
SPACE 
+  80mV 


STRIA 
VASCULARIS 


BLOOD 
■'  VESSELS 


SPIRAL 
LIGAMENT 


BONE 


FIG.  14.  Distribution  of  the  positive  endocochlear  potential'.  The  'endolymphatic  space'  of  the 
scala  media  is  shown  in  heavy  outline.  The  negative  intracellular  potentials  are  also  indicated.  The 
tectorial  membrane  is  omitted  and  only  one  external  hair  cell  is  shown.  [From  Tasaki  (16).] 


(in  anesthetized,  moribund  guinea  pigs).  Full  recovery 
requires  only  a  few  seconds  after  a  large  single  gasping 
inspiration.  It  is  also  abolished  rapidly  by  injection  of 
cyanide  or  azide  into  the  scala  tympani  or  scala 
media.  It  is  not  immediately  affected  by  injection  of 
isotonic  potassium  chloride,  choline  chloride,  or  po- 
ta.ssium  glutamate  into  the  scala  tympani  or  scala 
vestibuli.  It  does  fail,  although  less  rapidly  than  with 
cyanide,  following  surgical  injury  to  the  scala  media 
or  the  injection  into  the  scala  media  of  a  solution  that 
differs  substantially  in  ionic  content  from  the  analytic 
figures  for  endolymph  given  in  table  i. 

The  endocochlear  potential  is  modified  by  displace- 
ment of  structures  within  the  cochlear  partition.  Dis- 
placement of  the  basilar  membrane  toward  the  scala 
tympani,  as  by  injection  of  fluid  into  the  scala  media 
or  an  inward  movement  of  the  stapes,  causes  an  in- 
crease in  the  positive  potential  by  as  much  as  5  or  10 
mv.  Movement  in  the  opposite  direction,  as  by  out- 
ward movement  of  the  stapes,  causes  even  greater 
reductions  in  the  potential.  Movements  of  Reissner's 
membrane  alone  are  not  effective,  but  movements  of 
the  tectorial  membrane  relative  to  the  reticular 
lamina,  when  it  is  manipulated  by  a  microneedle, 
produce  just  such  changes  in  potential.  The  changes 


are  related  to  displacement,  not  to  velocity,  and  are 
sustained  as  long  as  the  displacement  is  maintained. 

The  source  of  the  endocochlear  potential  has  been 
identified  positively.  It  is  the  stria  vascularis  (6).  The 
changes  in  endocochlear  potential  described  above 
are  clearly  associated  with  the  organ  of  Corti,  almost 
surely  with  the  hair  cells,  but  the  resting  positive 
endocochlear  potential  is  not  generated  there.  Per- 
haps a  separate  electric  response  to  mechanical  move- 
ment occurs  in  the  hair  cells  and  simply  adds  to  the 
potential  that  is  produced  by  the  generator  in  the 
stria  vascularis,  or  perhaps  the  potential  of  scala 
media  is  modified  by  a  change  of  the  electrical  re- 
sistance to  the  continual  leakage  current  that  must 
flow  from  stria  \'ascularis  through  the  hair  cells. 

Cochlear  Microphonic  and  Sumrnatino  Potentials 

The  cochlear  microphonic  and  two  summating 
potentials  (positive  and  negative)  are  all  electric 
responses  to  acoustic  stimulation.  The  cochlear  micro- 
phonic is  linearly  proportional,  up  to  a  limit,  to  the 
displacement  of  the  cochlear  partition  and  thus, 
indirectly,  to  the  instantaneous  acoustic  pressure. 
The  microphonic  thus  reproduces  the  wave  form  of 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


577 


the  acoustic  stimulus  (fig.  15).  The  summatins;  poten- 
tials are  proportional  not  to  any  instantaneous  value 
of  the  acoustic  signal  but  to  a  root-mean-square  value, 
integrated  over  a  very  short  time.  Thus  the  summating 
potentials  reproduce  appro.ximately  the  form  of  the 
envelope  of  the  original  acoustic  signal.  The  positive 
and  the  negative  summating  potentials  are  opposite 
in  sign.  They  can  be  separated  by  the  greater  vulner- 
ability of  the  positive  summating  potential  to  oxygen 
lack  and  other  injury  and  by  the  more  apical  site  of 
generation  of  the  negative  response.  The  range  of 
linear  response  of  the  summating  potentials  has  not 
yet  been  determined. 

The  cochlear  microphonic  is  generated  at  the 
cuticular  surface  of  the  hair  cells.  This  is  clearly 
proved  by  exploration  with  microelectrodes.  The 
microphonic,  and  in  all  probability  the  summating 
potentials  also,  seem  to  reflect  the  bending  of  hairs  in 
the  appropriate  direction.  It  is  believed  that,  at 
intensities  high  enough  to  evoke  the  summating  poten- 
tials, some  kind  o*^  mechanical  rectifying  or  detector 
action  takes  place  in  the  inner  ear  to  cause  an  asym- 


BASAL 

250  TURN  n 

APICAL 


1000 


2000 


8000 


PAIRED    ELECTRODES,      SCALAE      VESTIBULI      AND     TyMPANI, 
IN     EACH       TURN 

FIG.  15.  Cochlear  microphonic  responses  to  'tone  pips'  of 
various  frequencies  recorded  simultaneously  from  the  basal,  the 
second  and  the  apical  turn.  The  wave  form  of  the  acoustic  sig- 
nals is  accurately  reproduced.  The  time  delay  (phase  difference) 
between  the  second  and  apical  turns  and  the  basal  turn  demon- 
strates the  traveling  wave  pattern.  The  failure  of  1000,  2000 
and  8000  cps  waves  to  reach  the  apical  turn  and  of  8000  cps  to 
reach  the  second  turn  demonstrates  acoustical  analysis.  The 
displacements  of  the  base  line  in  the  8000  cps  responses  are 
summating  potentials.  [From  Tasaki  (17).] 


metrical,  persistent  one-way  bend  in  the  hairs  of 
certain  cells.  In  some  cases  the  bending  is  probably 
across,  in  others  lengthwise  of,  the  organ  of  Corti. 
[A  theory  that  includes  this  and  several  other  aspects 
of  the  electrophysiology  of  the  cochlea  has  recently 
been  published  elsewhere  (4).] 

Both  the  cochlear  microphonic  and  the  summating 
potentials  are  continuously  graded  responses,  linearly 
related,  up  to  a  limit,  to  the  intensity  of  the  acoustic 
stimulus  and  with  no  true  '  threshold'  like  that  of  all- 
or-none  action  potentials.  No  evidence  of  anv  all-or- 
none  response  in  the  sen.sory  cells  or  of  a  refractory 
period  has  been  found,  even  when  the  cochlear  micro- 
phonic was  recorded  from  an  electrode  inside  a  hair 
cell.  Both  the  microphonic  and  the  summating  poten- 
tials show  very  little  or  no  fatigue  or  adaptation. 

The  cochlear  microphonic  'appears',  in  the  sense 
that  it  reaches  a  root-mean-square  value  of  a  micro- 
volt or  thereabouts,  at  a  much  lower  sound  pressure 
level  than  the  summating  potential  (except  at  the 
extreme  high-frequency  limit  of  response).  The  in- 
crease is  linear  with  the  sound  pressure  level  up  to 
about  90  db  relative  to  0.0002  microbar  in  the  guinea 
pig  but  varies  somewhat  with  frequency.  The  response 
then  increases  more  slowly  and  usually  goes  through  a 
maximum.  At  low  frequencies  harmonic  distortion 
(peak  limiting)  occurs  within  the  cochlea  at  even 
lower  levels  than  in  the  iTiiddle  ear.  For  high  fre- 
quencies, however,  the  sinusoidal  wave  form  of  the 
microphonic  is  maintained  even  when  the  increase  of 
amplitude  with  intensity  has  become  nonlinear.  This 
curious  behavior  is  in  sharp  contrast  to  the  peak 
limiting  seen  at  low  frequencies. 

The  summating  potentials  are  not  directly  related 
to  the  nonlinearity  of  the  mechanism  of  the  cochlear 
microphonics  although  they  may  happen  to  first  ap- 
pear at  sound  pressure  levels  which  lie  near  the 
beginning  of  nonlinearity.  With  increasing  intensity 
the  summating  potentials  do  not  reach  a  maximum 
but  continue  to  increase  up  to  limits  that  are  set  only 
by  acoustic  injury  to  the  organ  of  Corti. 

The  cochlear  microphonic,  like  the  endocochlear 
potential,  is  closely  dependent  on  an  adequate  oxygen 
supply.  In  anoxia  both  fall  almost  in  parallel,  and  the 
minor  differences  are  well  explained  by  the  changes  in 
electrical  resistance  of  Reissner's  membrane,  etc., 
which  also  occur  in  anoxia.  This  parallelism  is  a 
strong  argument  for  a  cau.sal  dependence  of  the  micro- 
phonic on  the  endocochlear  potential,  but  neverthe- 
less the  microphonic  may  also  be  abolished  by  certain 
injuries  that  leave  the  endocochlear  potential  un- 
affected. Two  such  injurious  procedures  are  a)  injec- 


578 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tion  of  a  high-potassium  solution  like  endolymph  into 
the  scala  tympani  (fig.  i6),  and  b')  poisoning  with 
streptomycin  just  sufficient  to  cause  degenerative 
changes  in  the  hair  cells. 

Surgical  injury  or  injections  into  the  scala  media  of 
solutions  of  ionic  makeup  substantially  different  from 
endolvmph  causes  a  depression  of  the  cochlear  micro- 
phonic as  well  as  the  endocochlear  potential,  as  in 
anoxia  but  more  slowly.  The  parallelism  between  the 
endocochlear  potential  and  cochlear  microphonic 
here  holds  in  general  but  is  not  exact. 

In  complete  anoxia,  and  continuing  for  an  hour  or 
more  post  mortem,  a  small  cochlear  microphonic 
remains.  This  residue  may  depend,  in  part  at  least, 
on  oxygen  that  diffuses  to  the  basal  turn  through  the 
round  window,  but  perhaps  it  is  generated  in  part 
by  an  anaerobic  mechanism.  This  latter  might  be  a 
transducer  action  like  that  of  a  condenser  microphone. 
The  primary  aerobic  cochlear  microphonic,  however, 
apparently  represents  an  amplifier  action  in  which 
energy  from  a  pre-existing  '  biological  pool  of  energy," 
such  as  is  suggested  by  the  endocochlear  potential, 
is  'valved'  or  modulated  by  the  mechanical  bending 
of  the  hairs.  In  any  case  the  sustained  changes  in  the 
endocochlear  potential  noted  above  certainly  repre- 
sent a  modulation  of  a  biological  source  of  energy  and 
not  simplv  a  passive  physical  transducer  mechanism. 

The  summating  potential,  as  usually  seen,  is  a  dis- 
placement of  the  base  line  of  the  oscilloscopic  record 
on  which  the  cochlear  microphonic  is  superimposed. 
It  is  a  paradoxical  fact  that,  with  mild  anoxia,  ab- 
normal ionic  concentrations,  etc.,  as  the  cochlear 
microphonic  diminishes  the  negative  summating 
potential  increases  (see  fig.  i6).  The  diphasic  effect 
of  anoxia,  etc.,  is  best  explained  by  assuming  a)  that 
there  is  a  positive  as  well  as  a  negative  summating 
potential,  generated  by  a  different  set  of  sensory  cells, 
and  ft)  that  the  positive  response  is  more  sensitive  to 
anoxia  than  the  negative.  The  negative  summating 
potential  seems  to  be  stronger,  although  higher  in 
'threshold'  (of  detection);  only  under  more  severe 
anoxia  or  ionic  injury  does  it  weaken  and  finally 
disappear.  The  positive  summating  potential  is 
attributed  to  the  inner  hair  cells  which  are  known  to 
be  the  more  sensitive  to  anoxia. "^  With  moderate 
stimuli  in  a  fresh  preparation  the  positive  summating 

'°  On  the  basis  of  more  recent  evidence  (6),  the  negative 
summating  potential  is  attributed  to  the  internal  hair  cells  and 
the  cochlear  microphonic  and  the  positive  summating  potential 
are  attributed  to  the  external  hair  cells. 


ENDO-TYRODE   IN  SCALA  TYMPANI 


Cochlear 

MlCROPHONIcl 
AND  .1 

Summating/ I 
Potential 

Action      / 
Potential 


STIMULUS 
4  MSEC 
9450  GPS 
I  MSEC 

RISE    TIME 


-."^J^^fNUlC^ 


BEFORE 
INJECTION 


10  SEC 
AFTER 


90  SEC 
AFTER 


DC    REMAINED  CONSTANT  at  75  MV 

FIG.  1 6.  A  solution  with  high  potassium  and  low  sodium 
concentration  like  endolymph  injected  into  the  scala  tympani 
depresses  cochlear  microphonic,  summating  potential  and  ac- 
tion potential  before  affecting  the  endocochlear  potentizd  in 
the  scala  media.  Note  the  transient  increase  in  summating 
potential  at  lo  sec.  Downward  deflections  indicate  the  scala 
vestibuli  to  be  more  negative  relative  to  the  scala  tympani 
{top  tines^  or  the  cochlea  more  negative  relative  to  the  neck 
(lower  lines').  [From  H.  Davis,  unpublished  observations.] 


Positive     SP 


Positive  and  Negative  SP 


0\^^r^ 


AP 

TONE     BURST 


AP 


8600       GPS 


+  20   DB 


FIG.  17.  Action  potentials,  cochlear  micophonics  and  sum 
mating  potentials  from  the  basal  turn.  An  increase  of  20  db  in 
stimulus  intensity  causes  the  negative  summating  potential 
nearly  to  obscure  the  smaller  positive  response.  Note  Ni,  No, 
and  Nt  in  the  action  potential  response  to  the  stronger  tone 
burst.  [From  H.  Davis,  unpublished  observations.] 


potential  may  dominate  (fig.  i  7)  The  reduction  in 
positive  summating  potential  causes  the  apparent 
increase  in  the  opposing  negative  summating  poten- 
tial. The  full  sequence  of  changes  may  be  even  more 
complicated  and  depends  in  its  details  on  the  initial 
condition  of  the  organ  of  Corti,  the  location  of  the 
recording  electrodes  and  the  frequency  of  the  tone 
bursts  used  to  elicit  the  responses. 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


579 


AUDITORY  NERVE   IMPULSES' 


Volleys  and  Latencies 

The  auditory  ner\e  responds  to  a  single  click  with  a 
sharp,  well-synchronized  volley  of  action  potentials, 
conventionally  designated  'Ni'.  If  the  click  is  of  moder- 
ate strength,  Ni  is  usuallv  followed  about  i  msec, 
later  by  a  smaller  second  vva\e,  'N)';  and  with  still 
stronger  clicks,  a  third  still  smaller  wave,  'Nj',  may  be 
seen  (fig.  17).  N5  and  N3  are  due  largely  to  repetitive 
firing  in  some  but  not  all  of  the  responding  fibers,  the 
interval  corresponding  to  the  refractory  period  of  the 
nerve  fibers. 

The  successive  sound  waves  of  a  steady  tone  of 
frequency  below  4000  cps  give  rise  to  similar,  although 
smaller,  volleys  of  action  potentials.  Between  4000 
and  2000  cps  the  indi\iduai  \'olleys  are  very  small, 
but  the  frequency  of  the  tone  is  nevertheless  clearly 
reproduced  in  the  action  potential  pattern,  even 
though  no  one  fiber  responds  to  every  sound  wave. 
This  pattern  of  occasional  but  synchronized  response 
to  a  regular  but  intermittent  stimulus  such  as  sound 
waves  is  the  basis  of  Wever's  (23)  'vollev  principle' 
(fig.  18). 

At  very  low  frequencies  in  the  guinea  pig,  both  Nj 
and  N2  may  sometimes  be  seen  in  response  to  each 
sound  wave,  but  both  are  rather  dispersed  in  time. 
At  1000  cps  and  below,  the  sharp  initial  portion  of  Nj 
is  initiated  in  the  lower  turns  of  the  cochlea  in  which 
the  partition  moves  almost  in  phase  as  a  unit.  The 
sharp  initial  'spike'  is  followed  by  a  more  diffuse 
'  tail'  of  impulses  from  the  inore  apical  regions. 

Not  only  do  different  fibers  have  different  latencies 
of  response  due  to  the  travel  time  of  the  traveling 
waves  but,  as  shown  by  studies  of  individual  fibers, 
the  latency  of  each  varies  from  one  response  to  the 
next.  This  variability  leads  to  a  less  and  less  perfect 
synchronization  of  the  impulses  as  the  frequency  is 
raised,  and  above  4000  cps  no  synchronization  is 
visible  on  the  oscilloscope  or  audible  by  ear.  At  the 
onset  of  a  high-frequency  tone  burst,  however,  there 
is  a  very  well  synchronized  Ni,  N2  and  perhaps  N3 
(figs.  16,  17).  The  latency  of  Ni,  the  sum  of  the  whole 
group  of  fibers,  is  very  stable  in  spite  of  the  variability 
among  individual  fibers.  The  latency  shortens  from  2 
msec,  or  more  near  threshold  to  about  i  msec,  as 
intensity  is  increased.  The  shortest  latency  reported  is 
0.55  msec.  The  latency  is  a  function  of  rise-time  as 

"  See  especially  the  papers  of  Davis  (4)  and  of  Tasaici 
(14.  15)- 


\AA/I\AA/ 


FIG.  18.  Single-fiber  spikes  in  two  different  fibers  of  the 
auditory  nerve  induced  by  1000  cps  pure  tones  about  55  db 
above  human  threshold.  Lower  Iracitig  is  sound  stimulus  recorded 
through  a  microphone.  Exposure  was  about  .015  sec.  [From 
Tasaki  (14).] 


well  as  the  intensity  and  perhaps  also  the  frequency  of 
the  acoustic  signal. 

The  latency  of  the  action  potentials  is  attributed 
chiefly  to  conduction  time  in  the  nonmedullated 
dendritic  iiranches  in  the  organ  of  Corti.  It  is  meas- 
ured from  the  beginning  of  the  cochlear  microphonic 
to  the  foot  of  the  action  potential  spike,  recorded  as 
the  volley  passes  through  the  modiolus.  (No  latency 
can  be  seen  between  mechanical  displacement  of  the 
cochlear  partition  and  the  cochlear  microphonic.) 

The  response  to  a  brief  high-frequency  transient 
such  as  a  click  or  the  onset  of  a  tone  burst  seems  to  be 
determined  by  the  wave-group  as  a  whole  as  if  a 
rectifier-detector  were  operating  in  the  ear.  The 
summating  potential  is  proijably  the  electrical  sign 
of  just  such  a  mechanical  detector  action. 

At  lower  frequencies,  below  about  3000  cps,  each 
sound  wave  acts  more  and  more  like  an  individual 
stimulus.  Excitation  apparently  occurs  during  the 
'falling  phase'  of  the  cochlear  microphonic,  i.e.  while 
the  .scala  media  (and  vestibuli)  is  ijecoming  more 
negative  relative  to  the  scala  tympani.  This  corre- 
sponds to  the  phase  of  outward  movement  of  the 
stapes.  The  latency  of  responses  to  individual  waves 
can  be  reckoned  consistently  and  logically  from  the 
positive  peak  (in  the  scala  media)  of  the  cochlear 
microphonic;  but  latency  measurements  are  compli- 
cated at  low  frequencies  because  of  the  progressive 
time  delay  of  the  travelinc:  wave. 

Single  Fiber  Activity 

Many  of  the  above  statements  concerning  latency 
of  response,    all-or-none    activity,    etc.,    of  auditory 


58o 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


nerve  impulses,  derived  originally  from  studies  of  the 
whole-nerve  action  potentials,  have  now  been  con- 
firmed or  extended  by  studies  of  single  fiber  activity. 

The  auditory  axons  are  2.5  to  4.0  fi  in  diameter. 
Tasaki  succeeded  in  inserting  hyperfine  electrodes 
into  individual  axons  in  the  modiolus  near  the  internal 
auditory  meatus  while  delivering  brief  tone  bursts  or 
steady  tones  to  the  guinea  pig  (fig.  18).  The  spike 
responses  resembled  those  from  myelinated  fibers  of 
similar  size  elsewhere.  Injury  discharge  was  some- 
times seen  and  also  responses  clearly  related  to  the 
auditory  stimuli.  The  response  to  a  brief  burst  or 
click  was  often  repetitive  as  shown  in  figure  i  g,  some- 
times outlasting  the  stimulus  by  20  to  30  msec.  The 
minimum  interval  between  impulses  in  such  discharges 
was  I  msec.  'Spontaneous'  impulses,  i.e.  not  correlated 
with  acoustic  stimuli,  were  often  seen  in  the  same  fibers 
that  also  gave  clear  responses  to  sounds.  No  inhibition 
of  spontaneous  impulses  by  acoustic  stimuli  was  ever 
seen. 

Some  fibers  regularly  tended  to  give  single,  others 
repetitive  responses.  Some  fibers  had  low  thresholds; 
others  high.  Most  fibers  were  partially  selective  with 
respect  to  frequency.  Each  showed  a  very  sharp  and 
very  stable  cut-off  frequency  above  which  it  failed  to 
respond  even  at  high  intensities  of  stimulation.  At  a 
frequency  only  slightly  below  the  cut-ofF  the  fiber  was 
most  sensitive,  but  the  rise  in  threshold  with  further 
reduction  in  frecjuency  was  very  gradual  (fig.  19). 
Nearly  all  fibers  encountered  had  cut-ofFs  above  1000 


Response  Area  of  a  single  auditory  nerve  fiber 


-      0 

> 


2  -20- 


3 

S  -40 


-60 


nil       11 

m 

III         111 

III             III 

/ 

III               III 

—    il          1 

■1 

II                IIDt 

III          m  1 

/ 

/ 

.11       II 

1 

II              III 

1           II 

/ 
1.         / 

^ 

^ 

\ 

/ 

,1  ' 

- 

1          -^ 

11/ 

1 

1          1 

1 

1               ' 

J 

1               1 

2345         6  7         8KC 

Frequency 


FIG.  19.  Repetitive  responses  of  a  single  auditory  fiber  to 
tone  pips  of  different  frequencies  and  intensities.  Doited  line 
shows  boundary  of  response  area'  of  this  fiber.  [From  Tasaki 
(■4)-] 


cps  but  in  a  few  fibers  a  cut-off  as  low  as  too  cps  was 
found.  The  'response  areas'  mapped  out  by  Tasaki 
are  much  like  those  described  earlier  by  Galambos 
and  Davis  for  units  now  known  to  be  second  order 
(cochlear  nucleus)  neurons;  but  the  high-frequency 
cut-off  is  rather  sharper,  the  low-frequency  decline  is 
more  gradual  and  inhibition  of  acoustic  responses  was 
never  observed. 

During  continued  tonal  stimulation  an  apparently 
irregular  discharge  continued  but  at  a  gradually 
diminishing  rate.  This  is  the  phenomenon  of  adapta- 
tion, for  which  there  is  also  good  psychoacoustic 
evidence.  The  continuing  discharge  was  superficially 
irregular  but  aciually,  except  for  a  few  (presumably 
'spontaneous')  impulses,  all  the  impulses  from  a  given 
fiber  fell  in  approximately  the  same  phase  relation  to 
the  tonal  stimulus  and  the  cochlear  microphonic  as 
explained  above. 

Concerning  recovery  from  adaptation,  fatigue,  or 
both,  the  information  from  psychoacoustics  has  con- 
siderably outrun  that  from  physiology.  The  recovery 
curve  of  Ni  of  the  composite  nerve  response  is  mono- 
tonic,  unlike  the  recovery  curve  of  psychoacoustic 
threshold.  The  partial  depression  of  a  second  nerve 
response  depends  both  on  the  intensity  of  the  first 
click  and  on  the  duration  of  the  interval  following  it, 
and  it  outlasts  by  10  msec,  the  refractory  period  of  the 
fillers. 

The  action  potential  threshold  for  clicks  in  guinea 
pig  or  cat  may  be  within  an  order  of  magnitude  of 
the  human  auditory  threshold.  With  increasing  power 
(in  decibels),  Ni  increases  along  a  sigmoid  curve, 
reaches  a  nearly  flat  plateau  and  then,  with  fairly 
strong  stimuli,  rises  much  more  rapidly  again.  The 
tendency  of  single  units  to  group  into  high -threshold 
and  low-threshold  classes  may  explain  this  nonlinear 
behavior  of  Ni  in  the  whole-nerve  response.  The  maxi- 
mum of  response  is  uncertain,  due  to  the  onset  of 
'fatigue'  or  'incipient  acoustic  trauma.' 

Efferent  Inhibitory  Aetion'- 

Stimulation  in  the  medulla  of  the  olivocochlear 
tract  of  Rasmussen  produces  an  inhibitory  effect  on 
the  action  potential  response  to  clicks.  Nj  is  clearly 
reduced,  but  the  cochlear  microphonic  is  not  affected. 
The  effect  is  very  specific  with  respect  to  the  location 
of  the  stimulating  electrodes,  and  the  middle  ear  with 
its  tympanic  muscles  is  definitely  not  involved.  The 
reduction  appears  some  20  to  30  msec,  after  stimula- 

'^  See  especially  the  paper  by  Galambos  (7). 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


581 


tion  has  begun  and  increases  up  to  about  250  msec. 
Rather  rapid  stimulation,  30  to  40  shocks  per  sec, 
is  required.  The  optimal  frequency  is  100  cps.  The 
long  latency  and  the  need  for  repetitive  stimulation 
show  quite  clearly  that  this  efferent  inhibitory  action 
is  not  related  to  the  temporal  priority  of  nearly  simul- 
taneous bilateral  signals.  It  is  apparently  an  expression 
of  a  rather  general  principle,  namely  central  regula- 
tion of  the  sensitivity  of  sense  organs.  The  functional 
relationships  of  this  reduction  in  sensitivity  are  com- 
pletely unknown. 


THEORY  OF  AURAL  ACTION''' 

This  author  has  suggested  elsewhere  a  series  of 
possible  mechanisms  and  interrelationships  that, 
taken  together,  offer  a  presently  tenable  working 
hypothesis.  This  theory  will  be  presented  here  in 
brief  for  its  value  in  unifying  many  varied  experi- 
mental observations,  but  the  reader  must  recognize 
that  several  assumptions,  interpretations  and  opin- 
ions, more  or  less  plausible,  are  now  added  to  the 
experimental  facts. 

Acoustic  energy  is  delivered  to  the  inner  ear  by  the 
external  and  middle  ears.  The  frequency  characteris- 
tics of  the  external  and  middle  ear  determine  to  a 
large  extent  the  shape  of  the  curve  of  auditory  sensi- 
tivity. The  impedance  match  provided  by  the  tym- 
panic membrane  and  ossicles  between  air  and  intra- 
cochlear  fluid  is  nearly  perfect,  except  perhaps  for 
frequencies  below  500  cps,  and  contributes  to  the 
great  absolute  sensitivity  of  the  ear.  Other  aspects  of 
the  middle  ear  structure  and  function  are  chiefly 
protective. 

Acoustic  pressure  on  the  tympanic  membrane 
causes  movement  of  the  foot-plate  of  the  stapes  and 
reciprocal  movement  of  the  round  window  membrane. 
The  fluid  movements  between  these  two  windows 
carry  with  them  the  elastic  cochlear  partition,  but  the 
pattern  of  movement  of  this  partition,  determined 
primarily  by  the  graded  stiffness  of  the  basilar  mem- 
brane, is  complicated.  The  pattern  is  a  sequence  of 
traveling  waves  that  move  very  rapidly  at  first,  then 
more  and  more  slowly,  as  they  travel  toward  the  apex. 
The  amplitude  increases  gradually  with  travel  to  a 
rather  flat  maximum  and  then  falls  off"  quite  sharply. 
The  positions  of  this  maximum  and  of  the  cut-off 
beyond  it  move  toward  the  apex  as  the  frequency  is 
reduced   and   toward    the   ba.se   as   the  frequency  is 

"  See  especially  tlie  papers  of  Davis  (3,  4). 


raised.  In  this  way  the  cochlea  acts  as  a  mechanical 
frequency  analyzer  and  the  'place  principle'  is  estab- 
lished as  one  element  contributing  to  frequency  dis- 
crimination. 

The  traveling  wave  pattern  is  an  expression  of 
pha.se  differences  in  the  movements  of  different  seg- 
ments of  the  cochlear  partition.  It  is  a  necessary  con- 
sequence of  the  graded  stiffness  of  the  cochlear  parti- 
tion, of  the  varying  ma.ss  of  fluid  that  moves  with  it 
and  of  the  rather  clo.se  coupling  inherent  in  a  continu- 
ous membrane  such  as  the  cochlear  partition.  The 
energy  is  transmitted  in  part  through  the  fluid  as  an 
acoustic  wave  and  in  part  along  the  membrane  from 
segment  to  segment.  The  stiffer  basal  region,  which 
for'  middle  and  low  frequencies  moves  almost  in 
phase,  tends  to  drive  the  more  flexible  apical  portion. 
The  nearly-in-phase  movements  of  the  partition  in  the 
basal  turn  in  response  to  low-frequency  sounds  cause 
nearly  synchronous  stimulation  of  impulses  in  many 
nerve  fibers.  Thus  the  frequency  principle  ('volley 
principle')  contributes  to  the  space-time  pattern  of 
nerve  impulses  in  spite  of  the  large  phase  differences 
that  are  a.ssociated  with  the  fundamental  traveling 
wave  pattern. 

The  movements  of  the  partition  in  the  traveling 
wave  pattern  involve  a  bending  of  the  basilar  mem- 
brane in  two  dimensions,  both  across  and  lengthwise. 
The  crosswise  bending  or  bulging  is  sharpest  at  the 
position  of  maximal  amplitude,  but  it  is  also  signifi- 
cant for  a  considerable  distance  basally  from  the 
ma.ximum.  The  longitudinal  l)ending  is  sharpest  in 
the  'cut-off'  region  on  the  apical  side  of  the  maximum 
and  is  probably  negligible  on  the  basal  side. 

As  the  basilar  membrane,  and  the  organ  of  Corti 
with  it,  bulge  one  way  or  the  other,  there  is  a  shearing 
action  between  the  stiff  reticular  lamina  of  the  organ 
of  Corti  and  the  stiff  and  viscous  tectorial  membrane 
that  lies  in  contact  with  it  becau.se  the  tectorial  mem- 
brane pivots  around  a  different  axis,  as  illustrated  in 
figure  12.  The  shearing  action  bends  the  hairs  of  the 
hair  cells,  which  are  attached  both  to  the  organ  of 
Corti  and  to  the  tectorial  membrane.  This  bending 
is  the  mechanical  movement  that  is  critical  for  stimu- 
lation. Protection  against  too  great  bending  probably 
is  provided  by  the  attachment  of  tectorial  membrane 
directly  to  the  outer  and  inner  borders  of  the  organ 
of  Corti. 

The  longitudinal  bending  causes  longitudinal 
vibratory  mov-ements  among  the  cells  of  the  organ  of 
Corti  and  presumably  bends  lengthwise  the  hairs  of 
the  cochlear  partition.  The  external  and  internal  hair 
cells  are  not  equally  sensitive   to  radial  and  longi- 


582 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOOY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tudinal  bending  of  iheir  hairs  (19a).  The  differential 
stimulation  of  the  two  sets  by  the  different  directions 
of  bending  allows  possibilities,  through  inhibitory 
neural  interactions  within  the  central  nervous  system, 
of  sharpening  the  'place'  aspect  of  frequency  discrimi- 
nation. 

The  large  traveling  waves  are  known  to  produce 
eddies  in  the  cochlear  fluids  on  the  apical  side  of  the 
position  of  maximal  amplitude.  The  forces  that  pro- 
duce eddies  we  beliexe  also  tend  to  cause  an  unsym- 
metrical  longitudinal  shift  or  'creep'  of  the  tectorial 
membrane  relative  to  the  organ  of  Corti.  .Such  a 
shift  would  cau.se  a  one-way  longitudinal  bending  of 
the  hairs.  This  is  a  mechanical  rectifying  action,  and 
it  allows  the  cochlea  to  'detect'  efficiently  and  respond 
with  nerve  impulses  to  high-frequency  acoustic  signals 
above  2000  per  sec.  Just  as  the  cochlear  microphonic 
is  the  electrical  sign  of  a  symmetrical  vibratory  bending 
of  the  hairs,  we  believe  the  negative  summating  poten- 
tial is  the  electrical  sign  of  an  asymmetrical,  rectified 
longitudinal  shift.  This  shift  is  strongest  on  the  apical 
side  of  the  position  of  ma.ximal  excursion.  The  sus- 
tained bend  of  the  hairs  presumably  acts  as  a  steady 
stimulus  to  the  hair  cells  that  are  affected,  but  com- 
pared to  the  alternating  shearing  movements  revealed 
by  the  cochlear  microphonic  this  mechanism  is  rela- 
tively insensitive.  The  rectifying  action,  as  revealed 
by  the  negative  summating  potential,  continues  to 
increase,  however,  after  the  vibratory  movements, 
and  with  them  the  cochlear  microphonic,  have 
reached  their  maximum.  The  rectifying  action,  no 
matter  how  it  is  produced,  seems  to  be  a  mechanism 
that  significantly  extends  the  dynamic  range  of  the 
ear. 

The  complete  theory,  as  presented  elsewhere,  con- 
siders the  mechanism  of  limitation  of  crosswise  bend- 
ing (and  with  it  the  cochlear  microphonic)  in  more 
detail  and  it  also  includes  a  second  rectifying  action, 
associated  with  the  crosswise  bending,  that  depends 
on  the  viscous  properties  of  the  tectorial  membrane. 
This  second  mechanical  rectifying  action  and  conse- 
quent one-way  bias  of  the  hairs  is  invoked  as  the  basis 
of  the  positive  summating  potential,  but  this  extension 
of  the  theory  as  well  as  a  possible  inhibitory  action  of 
the  positive  summating  potential  is  admittedly  more 
speculative  than  the  postulate  of  the  longitudinal 
"shift'  and  its  production  of  the  negative  summating 
potential. 

The  association  of  the  cochlear  microphonic  with 
the  bending  of  the  hairs  seems  very  well  established. 
The  mechanism  that  connects  the  two  is  completely 
obscure,    however.    A    vague    suggestion    that    the 


mechanical  distortion  changes  the  electrical  resistance 
of  the  upper  ends  of  the  hair  cells  has  been  offered  but 
without  supporting  evidence  (fig.  20).  Whatever  the 
mechanism,  the  bending  of  the  hairs  is  supposed  to 
account  for  not  only  the  cochlear  microphonic  but 
also  for  both  of  the  summating  potentials.  But  these 
three  electrical  responses,  it  should  be  noted,  are 
observed  phenomena,  not  theories. 

Consideration  of  the  extreme  .sensitivity  of  the  ear, 
and  afso  the  fact  that  the  summating  potential  per- 
sists indefinitely  if  a  static  displacement  of  the  tectorial 
membrane  relative  to  the  reticular  lamina  is  main- 
tained mechanically,  leads  to  the  conclusion  that  the 
energy  of  the  electrical  responses  is  derived  from  the 
metabolism  of  the  tissues,  not  from  the  acoustic 
stimulus.  The  latter  .serves  merely  to  'valve'  the  flow 
of  energy  from  the  biological  source.  The  result  is  an 
amplifier  action  in  the  sense  organ  prior  to  stimulation 
of  the  nerve  fibers. 

The  endocochlear  potential  has  been  hailed  as  the 
obvious  'pool  of  biological  energy'  that  is  tapped  by 
a  valving  action  of  the  hair  cells  (3).  Its  mechanism  is 
completely  unknown  but  it  seems  to  be  a  imicjue  prop- 
erty of  the  cochlea.  Its  analogue  in  the  utricle  is  not 


MODEL     OF 
COCHLEAR   EXCITATION 


"POLARIZED  RtLA^f' 
OR  OTHER  DETECTOR 
THAT  TRIGGERS  THE 
NERVE  IMPULSE 


FIG.  20.  An  electrical  model  of  excitation  of  nerve  impulses 
in  the  cochlea.  Additional  batteries',  not  shown  in  the  diagram, 
are  located  at  the  cell  membranes  of  the  hair  cells  and  of  the 
nerve  endings.  The  return  circuit  from  nerve  endings  to  the 
stria  vascularis  is  not  restricted  to  the  narrow  anatomical  path 
indicated  in  the  diagiam  but  is  diflfuse  through  all  intervening 
tissues  except  the  scala  media.  [From  Davis  (3).] 


EXCITATION    OF    AUDITORY    RECEPTORS 


5«:5 


more  than  5  mv  at  most.  It  is  dangerous,  therefore,  to 
ascribe  to  the  endocochlear  potential  anything  more 
than  an  accessory  function,  namely  to  hyperpolarize 
the  cuticular  surfaces  of  the  hair  cells  and  thereby 
increase  the  sensitivity  of  the  auditory  detector.  In 
the  utricle  the  negative  intracellular  potential  of  the 
hair  cells  apparently  must  suffice  as  the  '  pool  of  bio- 
logical energy.' 

The  unique  chemical  composition  of  endolymph 
does  not  necessarily  imply  a  high  positive  potential. 
The  high  potassium  is  present  in  the  utricle;  the  po- 
tential is  not.  The  two  are  probably  unrelated. 
Perhaps  the  high  potassium  merely  serves  to  maintain 
the  proper  colloidal  state  and  consequent  viscosity 
of  the  tectorial  membrane! 

The  cochlear  microphonic  and  the  negati\e  sum- 
mating  potential  are  believed  to  e.xcite  directly  the 
nerve  fibers  in  contact  with  the  hair  cells.  Only  a 
passive  role  as  electrical  conductors  is  ascribed  to  the 
nerve  endings.  There  is  no  synapse-like  delay  in 
excitation.  The  phase  relation  of  neural  excitation  to 
cochlear  microphonic  is  correct  for  the  electrical 
theory.  The  current  flows  from  hair  cell  into  nerve 
fiber  and  outward  across  the  nerve  membrane  and 
thus  can  e.xcite  the  nonmeduUated  dendritic  terminals 
like  one  tremendous  node  of  Ranvier  or  like  non- 
meduUated fibers  elsewhere.  Spatial  .summation  be- 
tween the  several  hair  cells  attached  to  a  given  nerve 
fiber  is  clearly  possible,  as  is  also  a  facilitating  action 
between  summating  potential  and  cochlear  micro- 
phonic. A  neurohumoral  step  between  hair  cell  and 
nerve  fiber  is  an  acceptable  addition  to  this  simple 
electrical  theory. 

Transmission  of  Auditory  Injorrnalion^'^ 

We  can  now  summarize  the  best  present  answers 
to  the  questions  implied  in  the  introduction  concern- 
ing frequency  and  intensity  discrimination  and  time 
differences. 

Frequency  (pitch)  discrimination,  the  core  of 
classical  'theories  of  hearing'  (11,  23)  is  now  con- 
sidered to  be  a  duplex  function.  We  do  not  think  of 
either  a  place  principle  (von  Helmholtz)  or  a  periodic- 
ity principle  (Rutherford)  but  of  a  combined  or  du- 
plex theory  (Wever,  Licklider). 

The  position  of  maximal  stimulation,  or  more 
probably  the  cut-off  boundary  of  strong  stimulation, 
is  certainly  one  part  of  the  mechanism  for  identifica- 

"  See  especially  the  papers  of  Davis  (4),  Licklider  (8),  von 
Bekesy  (21)  and  Wever  (23). 


tion  of  frequency,  particularly  of  high  frequencies. 
The  organ  of  Corti  of  the  basal  turn  is  essential  for  the 
hearing  of  high  tones.  Surgical  injuries  combined  with 
behavioral  tests  in  animals  and  disease  in  humans  have 
established  this  fact  firmly.  Partial  section  of  the 
auditory  nerve  may  cause  a  complete  high-tone  hear- 
ing loss.  Injuries  to  the  apical  end  of  the  cochlea  may 
cause  a  restricted  low-frequency  hearing  loss  but 
complete  loss  of  sensitivity  for  the  low  frequencies  does 
not  occur.  There  is  nevertheless  a  clear  relation  be- 
tween frequency  and  position  along  the  organ  of 
Corti.  Fine  frequency  discrimination  is  still  a  problem, 
however.  The  inaxima  of  the  '  resonance  curves'  of  the 
cochlear  partition  (fig.  8)  are  much  too  flat,  and  the 
'response  areas'  of  individual  nerve  fibers  (fig  19) 
are  too  asymmetrical  to  account  for  the  known  facts 
of  frequency  discrimination  without  some  additional 
hypothesis.  A  model  in  which  the  skin  of  the  forearm 
is  exposed  to  traveling  waves  of  tactile  stimulation  is 
surprisingly  effective,  however,  in  giving  a  sharp  suId- 
jective  location  of  the  tactile  sensation  and  in  dis- 
criminating changes  of  frequency  by  changes  in  this 
location  (21).  The  model  reinforces  the  general 
opinion  that  a  neural  interaction,  involving  inhibition 
of  the  impulses  from  less  strongly  stimulated  areas 
must  be  involved.  Such  inhibitory  interaction  at  the 
level  of  the  cochlear  nucleus  is  already  familiar. 

Direct  information  as  to  the  frequency  of  .sounds 
below  4000  cps  is  al.so  carried  in  the  auditory  nerve 
by  the  volley  principle.  This  information  is  believed 
to  contribute  importantly  to  frequency  discrimination 
and  to  the  sense  of  pitch  (8,  23).  Opinions  differ  as  to 
the  upper  frequency  at  which  it  ceases  to  be  important 
and  as  to  how  the  space  and  the  'periodicity'  prin- 
ciples interact  in  the  region  of  o\erlap.  In  any  ca.se 
the  periodicity  (volley)  principle  gains  in  importance 
and  the  place  principle  loses  as  the  frequency  is 
lowered. 

Intensity  discrimination  and  .subjective  loudness 
are  usually  attributed  to  the  number  of  nerve  impulses 
per  second  traversing  the  auditory  nerve.  Recruitment 
of  additional  fibers  as  intensity  is  increased  is  certainly 
one  mechanism  of  increasing  this  number,  and  faster 
average  rate  of  discharge  per  fiber  is  another.  It  is 
possiiale  also  that  certain  high-threshold  fibers  con- 
tribute more  per  fiber  to  loudness  than  do  others,  and 
it  is  by  no  means  necessary  to  assume  that  loudness  is 
a  simple  linear  function  of  the  total  number  of  im- 
pulses per  second. 

Temporal  information  and  also  the  binaural  differ- 
ences in   time   utilized   in   auditory   localization   are 


584 


HANDBOOK    t5F    PHYSIOLOGY  ^'  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


obviously  transmitted  in  the  form  of  time  differences 
between  volleys  of  impulses.  In  binaural  localization 
the  volleys  in  homologous  nerve  fibers  are  the  impor- 
tant ones.  The  very  small  time  differences  that  are 
known  to  suffice  show  that  there  is  a  statistical  con- 
stancy in  latencies,  conduction  times,  etc.,  that  is 
remarkable  in  view  of  the  variability  in  the  behavior 
of  the  individual  unit.  Here,  as  in  fine  discrimations 
in  general,  more  time  is  required,  often  with  repeated 


trials,  for  the  best  performance.  The  longer  time  al- 
lows for  more  complete  averaging  out  of  minor  vari- 
abilities. This  averaging  out  is  primarily  a  function  of 
the  central  nervous  system  rather  than  the  sense 
organ.  For  frequency  it  allows  very  fine  discrimination 
when  ample  time  is  allowed  or,  alternatively,  good 
discrimination  of  time  but  with  reduced  discrimina- 
tion for  pitch  when  the  duration  of  the  stimulus  is 
verv  short. 


REFERENCES'* 

1.  Barany,  E.  Acta  oto-taryng.  Suppl.  26,  1938. 

2.  Davis,  H.  (editor).  Hearing  and  Deafness:  A  Guide  for  Lay- 
men. New  York:  Rinehart,  1947. 

3.  Davis,  H.  In:  Physiological  Triggers  and  Disconlinuous  Rate 
Processes,  edited  by  T.  H.  Bullock.  Washington :  American 
Physiological  Society,  1957. 

4.  Davis,  H.  Physiol.  Ret'.  37:  i,  1957. 

5.  Davis,  H.,  R.  W.  Benson,  W.  P.  Covell,  C.  Fernandez, 
R.  Goldstein,  Y.  Katsuki,  J. -P.  Legouix,  D.  R.  Mc- 
AuLiFFE  AND  I.  Tasaki.  J.  Acoust.  Soc.  Am.  25:  1 180,  1953. 

6.  Davis,  H.,  B.  H.  Deather.\ge,  B.  Rosenblut,  C.  Fer- 
N.^NDEZ,  R.  Kimura  AND  C.  .\.  Smith.  Laryngoscope.  68: 
596,  1958. 

7.  G.-iLAMBOs,  R.  J.  Meurophysiol.  19:  424,  1956. 

8.  LicKLiDER,  J.  C.  R.  Experientia  7:  128,  1951. 

9.  LicKLiDER,  J.  C.  R.  In:  Handbook  oj  Experimental  Psy- 
chology, edited  by  S.  S.  Stevens.  New  York:  Wiley,  1951. 

10.  SnaxH,  C.  A.,  O.  H.  Lowrv  and  M.-L.  Wu.  Laryngoscope 
64:  141.  1954- 

11.  Stevens,  S.   S.   and  H.   Davis.   Hearing,  Its  Psychology  and 
Physiology.  New  York :  Wiley,  1 938. 

12.  Stevens,  S.  S.,  J.  G.  C.  Loring  and  D.  Cohen  (editors). 
Bibliography  on  Hearing.  Cambridge :  Harvard,  1 955. 

13.  Stuhlman,  O.  In:  An  Introduction  to  Biophysics.  New  York: 
Wiley,  1943. 


14.  Tasaki,  I.  J.  .Veurophysiol.  17:  97,  1954. 

15.  Tasaki,  I.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  19:  417,  1957. 

16.  Tasaki,  I.,  H.  Davis  and  D.  H.  Eldredoe.  J.  Acoust.  Soc. 
Am.  26:  765,  1954. 

17.  Tasaki,  I.,  H.  Davis  and  J. -P.  Legoui.x.  J.  Acoust.  Soc. 
Am.  24:  502,  1952. 

18.  von  Bekesv,  G.  Akust.  ^tschr.  6:  i,  1941. 

19    von  Bekesv,  G.  J.  Acoust.  Soc.  Am.  19:  452,  1947. 
iga.voN  Bekesv,  G.  J.  Acoust.  Soc.  Am.  25:  786,  1953. 

20.  VON  Bekesv,  G.  Ann.  Otol.  Rhin.  &  Laryng.  63 :  448,  1 954. 

21.  von  Bekesv,  G.  Science  123:  779,  1956. 

22.  VON  Bekesv,  G.  and  W.  .\.  Rosenblith.  In:  Handbook  of 
Experimental  Psychology,  edited  by  S.  S.  Stevens.  New  York : 
Wiley,  1 95 1. 

23.  Wever,  E.  G.   Theory  of  Hearing.  New  York:  Wiley,  1949. 

24.  Wever,  E.  G.  and  M.  Lawrence.  Physiological  Acomtics. 
Princeton:  Princeton  Univ.  Press,  1954. 

25.  ZwisLOCKi,  J.  Acta  oto-laryng.  Suppl.  72:  i,  1948. 

26.  Zv\tslocki,  J.  J.  Acoust.  Soc.  Am.  25:  743,  1953. 

''  Only  general  references,  a  few  key  papers,  and  the  sources 
of  the  figures  reproduced  in  this  chapter  axe  given.  Fairly  com- 
plete bibliographies  will  be  found  in  references  (4)  and  (12). 


CHAPTER     XXIV 


Central   auditory   mechanisms 


HARLOW"     W" .    A  D  E  S  U.  S.  .\'aval  School  of  Aviation  Medicine,  Pensacola,  Florida 


C:  H  A  P  T  E  R     CONTENTS 


INTRODUCTION 


Introduction 

Central  Auditory  Pathway 

Cochlear  Nuclei 

Efferent  Fibers  from  Cochlear  Nuclei 

Lateral  Lemniscus  and  Its  Nucleus 

Inferior  CoUiculus 

Inferior  Quadrigeminal  Brachium 

Medial  Geniculate  Body 

Auditory  Connections  with  Cerebellum 

Reticular  Activating  System 

Descending  Fibers  in  Auditory  Pathway 
Auditory  Cortex 

Auditory  Cortex  in  Primates 
Topologic  and  Tonotopic  Projection 

Summary  and  Discussion  of  Topologic  and  Tonotopic  Pro- 
jection 
Other  Aspects  of  Central  Auditory  Function 

Loudness 

Laterality  of  Projection 

Dispersion  of  Excitation,  Recurrent  Pathways  and  Inhibition 


THE  ASSIGNMENT  IN  THIS  CHAPTER  is  to  give  an  account 
of  the  cell  and  fiber  groups  of  the  brain  which  are 
more  or  less  directly  related  to  hearing.  The  coverage 
of  this  area  is  not  necessarilv  uniform  and  probably 
expresses  a  certain  degree  of  the  author's  bias,  not  only 
in  choice  of  material  but  perhaps  also  in  interpreta- 
tion. The  author,  adinitting  to  an  unspecified  degree 
of  bias  in  both  respects,  finds  it  futile  to  offer  apology 
but  instead  suggests  to  the  reader  the  additional  study 
of  a  recent  review  (32)  and  volume  (log)  to  which 
text  and  bibliographic  references  will  be  found.  The 
reference  listing  for  this  chapter  is  likewise  not  com- 
plete, especially  in  a  historical  sense,  and  the  reader 
may  remedy  this  deficiency  by  reference  to  the  works 
just  mentioned  and  also  to  the  recent  Bibliography  on 
Hearing  (98). 


The  history  of  research  on  the  central  auditory  path- 
way goes  back  only  a  few  years  into  the  last  century, 
not  even  as  far  as  the  study  of  other  sensory  systems. 
The  greater  volume  of  significant  work  has  been  done 
in  the  last  three  decades  only.  This  history  can  be 
divided  into  two  phases  which,  while  they  began  at 
different  times,  have  been  largely  concurrent.  The  first 
necessarily  concentrated  on  defining  the  neural  struc- 
tures which  are  primarily  concerned  with  sound 
stimulation  and  those  showing  fiber  connections,  more 
or  less  direct,  with  the  input  from  the  cochlear  nerve. 
The  early  functional  studies  suffered,  as  we  now  know, 
from  the  fact  that  the  pathway  of  projection  from  ear 
to  cerebral  hemisphere  is  bilateral  with  only  slight 
contralateral  preference;  consequently,  clinical  studies 
of  one-sided  damage  to  the  brain  produce  only  meager 
auditory  deficits,  and  these  can  be  demonstrated  only 
by  very  sensitive  tests.  For  this  reason,  studv  of  central 
auditory  inechanisms  lagged  behind  studies  on  other 
sensory  systems  in  which  there  is  a  great  preponder- 
ance of  contralateral  projection.  It  is  only  with  the 
advent  of  increasingly  reliable  physiological  appara- 
tus and  the  development  of  more  sensitive  behavioral 
testing  in  the  past  25  to  30  years,  that  tracts,  nuclei 
and  cortical  areas  responsive  to  auditory  stimulation 
have  been  reliably  defined.  This  definition  continues 
to  be  studied  and  revised,  although  attention  to  it 
has  gradually  given  way  to  studies  of  the  qualities  of 
response  rather  than  mere  presence  or  absence  of 
response. 

The  anatomical  aspect  of  the  first  phase  of  central 
auditory  study  fared  better  than  the  functional  and 
as  well  as  that  of  other  neural  systems.  The  original 
work  of  Ramon  y  Cajal  (75,  76),  as  far  as  the  auditory 
pathway  is  concerned,  has  been  amplified  and  em- 


585 


586 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


bellished  but  remains  basic.  The  cellular  and  fiber 
elements  we  traditionally  consider  as  components  of 
the  central  auditory  system  with  few  exceptions  are 
the  same  as  those  which  appear  in  Ramon  y  Cajal's 
diagrams. 

The  second  phase  of  research  on  the  central  audi- 
tory pathway  has  concerned  itself  with  the  discovery 
of  correlates  between  the  characteristics  of  sound 
stimuli  and  the  anatomicophysiologic  mechanisms 
activated  by  them.  From  the  beginning,  two  qualities 
of  sound,  frequency  and  intensity,  have  been  the  focal 
points  of  efforts  to  discover  the  neural  correlates  of 
hearing.  There  appear  to  be  several  interrelated 
reasons  for  this  and  for  the  consequent  preoccupation 
with  the  pure  tone  stimulus  in  auditory  research. 
Perhaps  the  most  compelling  force  has  been  the  in- 
fluence of  von  Helmholtz  who,  soon  after  the  middle 
of  the  nineteenth  century,  proposed  the  idea  that  the 
cochlea  functions  as  a  selectively  resonant  system  in 
which  tones  of  given  frequency  produce  localized 
resonance  of  the  basilar  membrane.  The  implication 
of  this  is  that  the  end  organ  functions  as  an  analyzer 
of  sound  and  delivers  to  the  brain  patterns  of  excita- 
tion which  are  already  analyzed  with  respect  to 
stimulus  frequency.  Besides  the  influence  of  von  Helm- 
holtz is  that  resulting  from  the  relative  ease  with 
which  pure  tone  stimuli  can  be  controlled  in  terms  of 
the  standard  parameters,  frequency  and  intensity. 
As  will  be  seen,  the  experimentalist's  bemusement 
with  pure  tone  has  had  both  an  advantageous  and 
disadvantageous  effect  upon  the  course  of  central 
auditory  research.  However  the  relative  good  and 
bad  may  be  evaluated,  it  is  impossible  to  discuss 
central  auditory  function  either  historically  or  cur- 
rently without  giving  a  great  deal  of  attention  to  pure 
tone  studies. 

There  has  been  in  recent  years  a  growing  trend 
away  from  pure  tone  studies  of  the  central  auditory 
system  (at  least  in  their  simplest  form).  There  are 
three  reasons  for  this:  /)  'Click'  stimulation  has  been 
frequently  used  (where  widespread  rather  than  selec- 
tive cochlear  stimulation  is  desired)  in  order  to  avoid 
the  experimental  consequences  of  the  frequency 
specificity  which  appears,  at  least  to  some  degree,  to 
be  characteristic  of  the  projection  pathway.  2)  There 
is  difficulty  in  studying  the  higher  auditory  centers 
by  postablational  hearing  tests  to  separate  the 
operated  from  the  unoperated  animals  unless  sound 
patterns  more  complex  than  pure  tones  or  auditory 
functions  requiring  more  than  acuity  and  frequency 
discrimination  are  used.  3)  There  is  a  feeling  among 
some  workers  in  audition  that  pure  tone  studies  ignore 


an  essential  temporal  clement  in  hearing  which  is 
introduced  when  clicks  or  other  complex  stimuli  are 
used. 

With  this  introduction,  we  can  proceed  to  examine 
the  available  data  relating  more  or  less  directly  to 
the  beginnings  of  neurological  explanation  of  some  of 
the  simpler  aspects  of  hearing  as  these  are  defined  in 
psychophysical  terms.  As  we  do  so,  it  will  become 
apparent  that  few  specific  questions  of  this  sort  can  be 
authoritatively  answered  at  this  time.  It  will  be 
further  apparent  that,  while  considerable  progress  in 
thinking  about  central  auditory  problems  has  occurred 
in  the  last  25  years,  few  of  even  the  earliest  studies 
during  this  period  are  altogether  obsolete,  although 
interpretations  and  conclusions  may  be.  Therefore, 
perhaps  the  best  approach  to  the  problem  will  be  a 
semihistorical  one  in  which  we  will  attempt  to  de- 
velop, summarize  and  evaluate  current  trends  of 
thinkine. 


CENTR.-\L    AUDITORY    PATHWAY 

To  define  as  central  auditory  mechanisms  all 
neural  elements  which  are  activated  by  stimulation 
of  the  organ  of  Corti  would  impose  an  impossible  task 
of  description  because  sound  stimulation  may  trigger 
neural  activity  of  far-reaching  systems  eventuating 
finally  in  the  activation  of  muscles.  Consequently, 
arbitrary  limits  must  be  imposed  on  the  description, 
if  possible  without  causing  a  corresponding  limitation 
on  our  thinking  of  the  con.sequences  of  sound  stimula- 
tion. One  might  choose,  for  example,  to  limit  the 
definition  of  central  auditory  mechanisms  to  the 
classical  pathway  of  Ramon  y  Cajal.  This  would 
include  fibers  and  nuclei  through  which  may  be 
traced,  anatomically,  a  clearly  sequential  series  of 
connections  from  the  ganglion  of  Corti  to  the  cerebral 
cortex:  cochlear  nuclei,  trapezoid  body,  superior 
olivary  nucleus,  lateral  lemniscus  and  its  nuclei, 
inferior  colliculus.  Inferior  quadrigeminal  brachium, 
medial  geniculate  body  and  its  fibers  radiating  to 
the  cortical  auditory  projection  area.  However, 
strict  adherence  to  the  classical  pathway  would  make 
it  impossible  to  explain  .several  phenomena  observed 
in  experiments  on  the  response  of  medial  geniculate 
body,  cerebral  cortex  and  cerebellum  to  soimd  stimu- 
lation. In  the  former  case,  for  example,  single  neural 
elements  of  the  medial  geniculate  body  may  be  found 
with  a  latency  of  response  far  too  long  to  be  accounted 
for  by  impulses  which  are  transmitted  via  the  tradi- 
tional pathway;  hence,  these  impulses  must  be  carried 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


587 


over  a  slower  system,  perhaps  one  not  hitherto  con- 
sidered as  auditory  (36).  A  similar  phenomenon  has 
been  demonstrated  in  the  cerebral  cortex  (25).  In  the 
case  of  the  ccrebelhim,  the  very  fact  that  response  to 
sound  may  be  evoked  requires  some  addition  to  the 
classical  definition  of  central  auditory  mechanisms. 
Finally,  while  they  have  been  mentioned  from  time  to 
time  for  manv  years,  fibers  coursing  within  the 
classical  pathway,  but  running  perversely  from  rostral 
to  caudal  instead  of  ascending,  have  been  until  re- 
cently consistently  ignored  as  functional  units. 

In  considering  the  tracts  and  nuclei  which  we 
classify  as  'central  auditory  mechanisms',  we  should 
keep  in  mind  several  functional  requirements,  some 
of  which  are  specificalK'  auditory  but  others  are  of 
more  general  neural  significance,  that  is  they  are 
functional  requirements  of  any  neural  system.  Taking 
up  the  general  requirements  first,  they  have  to  do  with 
two  closely  related  characteristics  of  neural  systems: 
/}  the  tendency  for  feed-back  devices  to  occur,  such  as 
recurrent  collaterals  by  which  any  neuron  mav  by  its 
own  discharge  feed  back  into  itself,  or  similar  mecha- 
nisms on  a  recurrent  nucleus-to-nucleus  basis,  this 
sort  of  device  apparently  serving  to  amplify  the  effect 
of  input  into  the  system;  and  1^)  the  apparent  ability  of 
the  system,  by  its  own  activity,  to  modify,  modulate 
or  control  that  activity,  a  function  which  could  be 
served  by  the  same  kind  of  recurrent  feed-back  circuits. 

The  more  specifically  auditory  requirements  have 
to  do  /)  with  the  mechanisms  by  which  sound  is 
analyzed  with  respect  to  frequency  and  intensity,  and 
the  combinations  and  permutations  of  these,  and  the 
manner  in  which  these  are  impressed  upon  the 
brain,  and  2)  with  the  arrangements  by  which  the 
activities  of  the  auditory  system  impress  themselves  on 
integrative  mechanisms  of  the  brain  and  ultimately  on 
motor  systems  through  which  responses  to  auditory 
stimulation  may  be  mediated.  It  has  been  common  to 
speak  of  this  kind  of  function  in  terms  of  '  levels  of 
integration',  as  though  integration  of  auditory  infor- 
mation could  be  classified  as  to  its  own  complexity 
and  to  the  complexity  of  response  called  for,  and  each 
category  relegated  to  a  particular  rostrocaudal  level. 
This  concept  may  have  a  very  general  kind  of  validity, 
but  it  will  become  increasingly  evident  that  its  useful- 
ness is  questionable.  It  must  be  applied  with  great 
caution,  and  the  delegation  of  degrees  of  perceptual 
judgment  to  one  or  another  of  the  cell  masses  of  the 
central  auditory  pathway  represents  a  pattern  of 
thinking  which  is  more  dangerous  than  helpful. 

There  appear  to  be  significant  differences  between 
the  pathway  of  projection  from  sensory  end  organ  to 


cerebral  cortex  in  the  auditory  system  as  compared 
with  other  afferent  systems,  for  example  the  somatic 
sensory  systems.  The  latter  are  characterized  by  a 
second-order  link  with  a  thalamic  nucleus,  whereas 
in  the  auditory  the  fibers  reaching  the  thalamus  are 
at  least  third  order,  and  there  are  probably  relatively 
few  that  are  not  of  fourth,  fifth  or  higher  order.  There 
is,  in  other  words,  in  the  auditory  pathway  a  more 
complex  and  devious  system  of  nuclear  interruptions. 
One  factor  leading  to  this  situation  must  have  been 
that,  since  the  cochlea  and  its  central  connections  de- 
veloped phylogenetically  late  as  compared  with  other 
sensory  .systems,  the  ascending  pathway  had  to  be 
constituted  from  such  scattered  elements  as  were  still 
open  to  modification  in  a  neural  matrix  otherwise  too 
fixed  in  pattern  to  permit  of  a  new  through  pathway. 
Figure  i  shows  diagrammatically  the  main  features 
of  the  known  connections  of  the  auditory  pathway. 
Cerebellar  connections  are  not  shown.  Connections 
with  the  reticular  system  are  shown  schematically. 
These  are  actually  not  known  in  anatomical  detail, 
but  some  .such  connections  must  be  present  according 
to  physiological  evidence. 

Cochlear  Nuclei 

The  course  and  terminations  of  two  types  of  den- 
dritic processes  of  cells  of  the  spiral  ganglion  of  Corti 
have  been  studied  and  described  by  several  authors 
(e.g.  28,  59,  75).  Despite  the  rather  elaborate  differ- 
entiation thus  revealed  in  the  end  organ  of  hearing, 
the  course  and  terminations  of  the  axons  of  the 
ganglion  cells  show  no  such  corresponding  differenti- 
ation; rather,  the  terminations  display  a  pattern  of 
organization  of  a  different  sort.  For  practical  purposes 
then,  our  story  of  the  central  auditory  pathway  may 
begin  with  a  group  of  fibers,  showing  little  differenti- 
ation, entering  the  medulla  at  the  inferior  border  of 
the  pons  as  the  cochlear  portion  of  the  eighth  cranial 
nerve.  Immediately  the  fibers  begin  to  bifurcate  and 
the  resultant  branches  to  pass  to  their  terminations  in 
dorsal  and  ventral  cochlear  nuclei  (28,  75).  Each 
fiber  is  said  to  terminate  on  75  to  too  cells  of  the  coch- 
lear nuclei.  This  being  true,  it  must  also  follow  that 
each  cell  of  the  nucleus  receives  terminations  from 
many  incoming  fibers  because  the  total  number  of 
cochlear  nucleus  cells  is  only  about  2.9  times  the  num- 
ber of  cells  of  the  ganglion  of  Corti  (22). 

The  cochlear  nuclei  are  divisible  each  into  several 
parts.  The  organizational  pattern  in  the  dorsal  coch- 
lear nucleus  is  laminar,  that  in  the  ventral  is  not  but 
shows  a  similar  degree  of  complexity  and  differenti- 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOOY    I 


FIG.  1.  Main  features  of  the  known  connections  of  the  auditory  pathways  in  the  cat.  .-1,  medial 
geniculate  body;  B,  superior  colliculus;  C,  inferior  coliiculus;  D,  cochlear  nucleus;  E,  superior  olive; 
F,  cut  section  of  brachium  pontis;  2,  corticopontocerebellar  pathway;  3,  recurrent  fibers  throughout 
the  auditory  projection  pathway;  4,  commissure  of  Probst;  j,  brachium  of  inferior  colliculus;  6, 
commis.sure  of  inferior  colliculus;  7,  nucleus  lateral  lemniscus;  8,  lateral  lemniscus;  9,  olive  cochlear 
bundle;  ;o,  cochlear  nerve;  //,  trapezoid  body;  12,  reticular  system  (diffuse  projection  to  cerebral 
cortex). 


ation.  Both  contain  many  cell  types  (28,  68)  and 
several  types  of  axon  endings.  Of  the  former,  some 
are  recognized  as  intranuclear  short  axon  cells  (28). 
In  the  light  of  present  knowledge,  there  is  little  evi- 
dence to  tell  us  what  may  be  the  functional  signifi- 
cance of  this  coinplcx  organization  or  even  its  signifi- 


cance in  terms  of  distribution  of  efferent  fibers  from 
the  cochlear  nuclei.  Rose  et  al.  (84)  have  recently 
demonstrated  a  functional  organization  in  terms  of 
frequency  but  have  not  yet  tied  this  firmly  to  the 
histological  pattern.  For  the  present,  therefore,  we 
have  little  choice  l)ut  to  ignore  most  of  the  organiza- 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


589 


tional  features  of  cochlear  nuclei  in  describing  the 
efferent  fibers  which  leave  them,  particularly  as  the 
latter  display  a  very  great  range  of  diameters  (75). 

Efferent  Fibers  from  Coe/ileiir  .Suelei 

Three  principal  groups  of  fibers  emanate  from  the 
cochlear  nuclei  (13,  60).  /)  The  dorsal  (or  superior) 
acoustic  stria  leave  the  dorsal  nucleus  to  pass  through 
the  reticular  formation  under  the  medial  longitudinal 
fasciculus,  and,  upon  crossing  the  mid-line,  pass 
ventrolaterally  to  the  vicinity  of  the  superior  olivary 
nucleus.  1^)  The  intermediate  stria  arises  from  the 
dorsal  part  of  the  ventral  cochlear  nucleus,  passes 
over  the  restiform  body  and  crosses  the  reticular 
formation  to  the  opposite  side  in  an  intermediate 
position,  j)  Fil:)ers,  which  e.xceed  in  number  the  com- 
bined total  of  the  other  two  striae,  arise  in  the  main 
body  of  the  ventral  cochlear  nucleus,  pass  directly 
medially  \cntral  to  the  restiform  body,  traverse  the 
ventral  part  of  the  reticular  formation  and  cross  the 
mid-line  as  the  trapezoid  body  (or  \'entral  stria). 

The  three  striae  tend  to  draw  together  in  the  \icin- 
ity  of  the  contralateral  superior  olivary  nucleus  where 
many  of  them  terminate.  In  the  course  of  their  passage 
from  origin  to  that  point,  there  is  a  considerable  dimi- 
nution of  fibers  even  before  the  mid-line  is  crossed 
due  to  termination  of  some  fibers  in  the  reticular 
formation  and  others  in  the  ipsilateral  superior  olivary 
nucleus  (13).  The  latter  are  of  .special  interest  insofar 
as  they  provide  an  essential  part  of  an  anatomical 
basis  for  ipsilateral  rostral  projection  and  possibly  for 
reflex  connections  at  the  medullary  level. 

A  few  fibers  emanating  from  the  cochlear  nuclei 
fail  to  be  interrupted  by  synapses  in  either  the  ipsi- 
lateral or  contralateral  superior  olivary  nucleus  (13) 
but  turn  rostrally  and  ascend  through  the  pontine 
medulla  in  company  with  third  order  fibers  which 
arise  from  the  superior  olivary  nucleus,  the  combined 
elements  being  called  the  lateral  lemniscus.  Appar- 
ently all  of  the  second  order  fibers  which  ascend  in  the 
contralateral  lateral  lemniscus  terminate  in  either 
the  nucleus  of  the  lateral  lemniscus  or  the  inferior 
coUiculus  (13). 

Lateral  Lemniseus  and  its  Nueleus 

This  tract  ascends  from  the  region  of  the  superior 
olivary  nucleus  to  the  inferior  colliculus  and,  in  part, 
beyond,  as  the  inferior  quadrigeminal  brachium,  to 
the  medial  geniculate  body  in  the  thalamus.  Between 
superior  olive  and  inferior  colliculus,  the  tract  is  com- 


posed of  at  least  two,  and  probably  more,  different 
components,  /)  fibers  having  origin  in  the  contra- 
lateral cochlear  nuclei  (13)  and  2)  fibers  taking  origin 
from  the  ipsilateral  superior  olivary  nuclei  (13,  33, 
69,  71.  75)-  It  should  be  noted  that  since  the  superior 
oli\e  receives  second  order  fibers  from  both  ipsi-  and 
contralateral  cochlear  nuclei,  the  two  components  of 
the  lateral  lemniscus  listed  above  can  actually  be 
sui^divided  into  three  with  respect  to  cochlear  origin 
of  excitation  carried  by  each:  /)  contralateral  second 
order,  2)  contralateral  third  order  and  3)  ipsilateral 
third  order. 

At  this  point  it  may  be  pointed  out  that  our  use  of 
'second  order'  and  'third  order'  is  valid  only  if  we 
assume  a  single  synapse  in  each  successive  nucleus, 
for  each  chain  of  conduction  as  represented  at  one 
single  point  by  a  fiber  of  the  lateral  lemniscus.  This 
a.s.sumption  is  neither  necessary  nor  likely  in  view  of 
the  complexity  of  the  nuclei  so  far  encountered.  It 
would  seem  more  likely  that  a  variable  number  of 
links  in  such  chains  of  conduction  might  be  introduced 
by  the  patterns  of  intranuclear  conduction.  Evidence 
on  conduction  time  to  the  cochlear  nucleus,  trapezoid 
body  and  lateral  lemniscus  (5)  indicates  that  there  are 
at  least  some  conduction  chains  in  the  system  which 
are  as  direct  as  would  be  implied  in  speaking  of  second 
and  third  order  fibers  in  the  lemniscus;  however,  the 
protraction  of  the  response  to  a  very  brief  stimulus 
would  also  make  one  suspect  the  presence  of  chains 
with  greater  numbers  of  synapses. 

The  nucleus  of  the  lateral  lemniscus,  unlike  the 
other  nuclei  so  far  discussed,  is  neither  compact  nor 
does  it  show  any  recognizable  organization.  It  con- 
sists of  scattered  groups  of  cells  lying  among  the  fibers 
of  the  tract.  Some  few  of  the  tract  fibers  apparently 
terminate  in  synapse  with  these  cells,  and  in  turn  they 
send  their  axons  upward  with  the  tract  to  termina- 
tions in  the  inferior  colliculus,  probably  both  ipsi- 
lateral and  contralateral  (by  way  of  the  commissure 
of  Probst). 

Some  fibers  of  the  lateral  lemniscus,  of  third  order 
or  higher,  pass  lateral  to  the  inferior  colliculus  and, 
becoming  part  of  the  inferior  quadrigeminal  bra- 
chium, continue  with  it  to  terminations  in  the  medial 
geniculate  body  (3,  51,  75).  The  greater  number  of 
lemniscal  fibers  terminate  in  the  inferior  colliculus 
(13.  75)- 

Inferior  Colluulus 

The  inferior  colliculus  (or  posterior  corpus  quadri- 
geminum)  receives  a  few  fibers  which  project  without 


590 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


interruption  from  the  contralateral  cochlear  nuclei, 
many  more  from  the  ipsilateral  superior  olivary 
nucleus  and  a  few  (proiaably)  from  the  nucleus  of  the 
lateral  lemniscus,  ipsi-  and  contralateral.  The  input 
to  this  nucleus,  therefore,  represents  a  degree  of  diver- 
sity and  temporal  dispersion  still  greater  than  that  of 
the  superior  olive  and  cochlear  nuclei.  This  is  demon- 
strated by  the  relatively  greater  protraction  of  re- 
sponse to  brief  stimuli  than  is  seen  in  the  more  caudally 
located  stations  of  the  pathway  (5,  90). 

The  inferior  colliculus  is  one  of  the  most  highly 
organized  and  largest  nuclei  of  the  brain  stem.  In- 
deed, its  position,  size,  pattern  of  organization  and 
multiplicity  of  afferent  and  efferent  connections  would 
make  it  seem  more  logical  to  consider  it,  together  with 
the  superior  colliculus,  suprasegmental  rather  than  a 
brain-stem  nucleus  in  the  usual  meaning.  At  any  rate, 
the  organization  and  fiber  connections  are  such  that, 
despite  the  fact  that  the  inferior  colliculus  must  func- 
tion to  some  extent  as  a  relay  in  the  ascending  audi- 
torv  pathway,  its  significance  can  by  no  means  be 
limited  to  its  relay  function.  This  will  be  discussed 
further  in  a  different  context  in  a  later  section  of  this 
chapter. 

Inferior  Qiiadrigermnal  Braclnum 

The  colliculus,  in  addition  to  efferent  pathways  to 
superior  colliculus  and  pons  (77),  has,  as  its  principal 
route  of  discharge,  the  brachium  of  the  inferior  collic- 
ulus (or  inferior  quadrigeminal  brachium).  This 
tract  is  composed  predominantly  of  fibers  arising  in 
both  ipsilateral  and  contralateral  colliculi,  those  from 
the  latter  passing  through  the  commissure  of  the  in- 
ferior colliculus  (ill).  In  addition,  there  is  present 
in  the  tract  the  group  of  lemniscal  fibers,  noted  in  the 
preceding  section,  that  bypasses  the  colliculus.  The 
entire  brachium  passes  rostrally  and  .somewhat  later- 
ally to  terminate  in  the  medial  geniculate  body. 

Medial  Geniculate  Body 

The  medial  geniculate  body  is  the  thalamic  nucleus 
of  the  auditory  pathway.  It  is  described  as  having  a 
pars  principalis  compo.sed  of  small  closely-packed 
cells  arranged  in  a  laterodorsally  curving  band,  and 
a  pars  magnocellularis,  lying  medioventral  to  the 
pars  principalis  and  composed  of  large  cells  (10,  23). 
There  is  some  doubt  that  the  magnocellular  part 
should  be  considered  a  part  of  the  true  auditory 
thalamic    relay,    although    the    terminology    which 


makes  it  a  part  of  the  medial  geniculate  has  been 
generally  accepted  for  many  years. 

The  pars  principalis  appears  to  be  fairly  homogene- 
ous with  respect  to  cell  size  and  distribution,  except 
for  a  slightly  decreasing  gradient  of  density  from  lat- 
eral to  medial  aspects  (85).  Thus,  there  is  none  of  the 
conspicuous  organizational  complexity  of  the  brain- 
stem acoustic  nuclei.  The  principal  input  to  the  nu- 
cleus consists  of  the  terminations  of  the  inferior 
quadrigeminal  brachium.  Other  than  these,  the  only 
fibers  reported  as  afFerents  to  the  nucleus  are  recurrent 
projections  from  the  cortical  projection  area  (62). 
Aside  from  a  small  number  of  fillers  which  are  dis- 
tributed rather  diffusely  to  other  parts  of  the  thalamus 
(i)  and  a  few  which  retrace  the  lower  projection  path- 
way (i),  the  main  efferent  outflow  from  the  medial 
geniculate  is  the  acoustic  radiation.  These  fibers 
proceed  bv  way  of  the  posterior  limb  (sublenticular 
portion)  of  the  internal  capsule  to  part  of  the  superior 
face  of  the  superior  temporal  gyrus  and  adjacent 
insular  and  parietal  opercular  cortex  in  primates  and 
the  corresponding  cortex  in  carnivors  which  lack  a 
true  temporal  lobe.  Discussion  of  the  projection  areas 
forms  the  subject  matter  of  a  later  section  of  this 
chapter. 

As  noted  above,  the  medial  geniculate,  pars  princi- 
palis, shows  little  or  no  histologically  demonstrable 
organization;  however,  there  is  other  evidence  indicat- 
ing that  there  is,  nevertheless,  at  least  a  spatial  type 
of  organization.  This  is  inseparable  from  evidence  of 
similar  organization  in  other  parts  of  the  auditory 
system  and  a  separate  section  will  be  devoted  to  spa- 
tial and  tonotopic  aspects  of  the  projection  pathway. 

Auditory  Connections  with  Cerebellum 

.Snider  &  .Stowell  (95)  in  1944  reported  the  hitherto 
unknown  fact  that  auditory  stimuli  (clicks)  could 
regularly  evoke  responses  from  the  cortex  of  the 
cerebellar  \ermis  in  cats.  In  subsequent  experiments 
these  findings  were  confirmed  and  the  additional  dis- 
covery made  that  stimulation  by  light  flashes  also 
elicits  response  in  the  same  cerebellar  area.  The 
responses  to  auditory  stimulation  occur  with  latency 
so  brief  as  to  imply  a  fairly  uncomplicated  projection 
from  the  periphery.  At  the  time  of  the  original  ob- 
servations, no  such  cochleocerebellar  path  was  known. 
Since  then  Niemer  &  Cheng  (68)  have  deduced  the 
existence  of  a  pathway  by  which  the  ventral  part  ot 
the  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus  sends  fibers  to  termina- 
tions in  the  cerebellar  \ermis.  Their  evidence  consists 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


59' 


in  the  observation  of  retrograde  chromatolysis  in  that 
nucleus  as  a  consequence  of  destruction  of  the  vermis. 
The  tectopontile  tract  described  by  Rasmussen  (77), 
which  provides  communication  from  the  inferior 
colHculus  to  the  pons,  would  also  seem  a  possible 
avenue  from  auditory  pathway  to  the  cerebellum. 

What  may  be  the  functional  significance  of  such  a 
system  is  a  proper  but  as  yet  unanswered  question. 
The  further  information  that  stimulation  of  the 
audiovisual"  area  of  the  cerebellum  may  evoke  re- 
sponse in  the  cerebrocortical  auditory  area  (94)  and 
that  stimulation  of  the  latter  elicits  response  from  the 
former  (42)  may  offer  some  help  in  answering  the 
question.  One  suggestion,  which  has  been  made 
repeatedh'  by  one  of  the  more  ardent  advocates  of  a 
cerebellar  contriljution  to  audition,  is  that  the  coch- 
leocerebellocerebral  pathwas  pro\ides  an  alternative 
pathway  of  auditory  projection  or  integration  (or 
both)  to  the  cerebral  cortex  which  inay  be  implicated 
in  the  preservation  of  auditory  function  after  inter- 
ruption of  the  regular  cortical  projection  pathway. 
This  is  neither  a  necessary  nor  a  likely  hypothesis. 
The  fact  that  the  cerebellum  receives  an  auditory 
projection  does  not  imply  that  it  is  implicated  in  the 
psychological  phenomenon  of  audition  per  se. 

The  cerebral  connections  to  the  cerebellum  are 
presumably  those  described  b\'  Mettler  (62)  as  pro- 
jecting from  the  cat's  cerebral  auditory  area  to  the 
pons  froiTi  which  a  pontocerebellar  relay  would  be 
the  expected  pattern.  A  cerebellocerebral  pathway 
from  the  cerebellar  cortex  to  the  cerebellar  nuclei  to 
the  thalamus  to  the  cortex  would  be  a  plausible  or 
even  probable  explanation  of  the  functional  evidence 
that  the  auditory  area  of  the  cerebellum  projects  to 
that  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  It  is  the  more  plausible 
when  we  recall  the  similar  type  of  anteriorly  directed 
projection  of  the  brachium  conjunctivum.  The  pattern 
of  interprojection  of  cerebellar  and  cerebral  areas  is 
thus  probably  no  different  in  relation  to  the  auditory 
than  to  any  of  several  other  functional  systems.  The 
most  likely  explanation  of  cerebellar  auditory  (and 
visual)  connections,  therefore,  would  seem  to  be  that 
these  add  the  information  of  distance  receptors  to  that 
of  contact  and  proprioceptive  receptors  as  these  may 
modulate  the  cerebellar  contribution  to  regulation  of 
motor  patterns. 


sumably  at  brain-stem  levels.  Certainly,  it  has  been 
demonstrated  that  arousal  can  be  induced  by  auditory 
stimuli  in  animals  in  which  the  standard  acoustic 
projection  pathway  has  been  bilaterally  interrupted 
(30,  57,  61,  96,  97).  Thus,  there  is  through  the  retic- 
ular system  another  route  from  brain-stem  acoustic 
mechanisms  to  cerebral  cortex,  though  this  is  of 
general  rather  than  specifically  auditory  distribution. 
The  ascending  reticular  system  .seems  to  be  a  diffuse 
and  multisynaptic  route,  so  the  auditory  and  other 
specific  modalities  of  input  tend  to  be  swallowed  up 
in  the  more  comprehensive  functions  of  the  ascending 
reticular  system.  It  is  impossible  to  say  to  what  ex- 
tent, if  any,  this  system  may  serve  a  specific  sensory 
function,  though  it  would  appear  that  this  could  not 
be  extensive  in  the  light  of  what  we  know  about 
reticular  function. 

Descending  Fibers  in  the  Auditory  Pathway 

Fibers  which  proceed  from  rostral  to  caudal  re- 
gions, that  is  from  higher  to  lower  stations  in  the 
auditory  projection  pathway,  have  been  described  at 
all  levels  from  the  cerebral  cortex  to  the  cochlea.  In 
general  these  closely  parallel  the  ascending  system, 
although  they  may  bypass  nuclei  with  greater  free- 
dom. They  are  better  known  anatomically  than 
physiologically  with  the  possible  exception  of  the 
olivocochlear  tract  which  was  described  by  Rasmus- 
sen  (78-80).  For  a  review  of  the  available  evidence 
and  current  studies  of  the  descending  auditory  path- 
ways, the  reader  is  referred  to  Chapter  XXXI  by 
Livingston  in  this  volume  and  to  Galambos'  recent 
review  (32,  p.  502).  He  indicates  that,  for  the  first 
time,  a  systematic  anatomic  study  of  these  by  ade- 
quate degeneration  methods  is  under  way.  While  the 
information  is  as  yet  meager,  it  is  clear  that  a  neural 
system  which  provides  a  possible  mechanism  by  which 
the  sensory  system  to  which  it  belongs  can  achieve 
some  degree  of  .self-regulation  may  be  of  the  utmost 
importance  in  providing  explanation  of  complex 
functions,  the  means  for  which  are  not  obviously 
available  in  the  organization  of  the  afferent  pathway. 


.AUDITORY    CORTEX 


Reticular  Activating  System 

Like  other  sensory  systems,  the  acoustic  makes  its 
contribution  to  the  reticular  activating  system  pre- 


The  development  of  knowledge  of  the  cortical  ter- 
mination of  the  auditory  projection  system  may  be 
said  to  have  begun  with  the  observations  of  Ferrier 
(29)  reported  as  part  of  a  more  general  work  in  1876. 


592 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Observations  of  the  responses  of  cats  to  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  brain,  such  as  movements  of  the 
ears  and  turning  of  head  and  eyes,  led  Ferrier  to 
identify  as  auditory  in  function  the  ectosylvian  region. 
This  area  has  remained  the  '  auditory  area'  ever  since, 
although  the  exact  limits  of  the  cortex  so  designated 
have  varied  considerably  with  variations  in  method, 
investigator  and,  presumably,  also  with  variation  in 
the  cat  itself.  For  if  there  is  one  single  incontrovertible 
fact  which  has  emerged  from  a  long  series  of  investi- 
gations of  this  area,  it  is  that  a  too  faithful  reliance 
on  the  correspondence  between  visible  brain  markings 
and  functional  significance  in  this  or  any  other  region 
in  this  or  any  other  animal  is  a  trap  for  the  unwary. 

Ferrier's  observations  apparently  satisfied  everyone 
for  about  20  years  because,  for  that  period  of  time,  no 
other  work  appeared,  either  to  contradict  or  to  modify 
Ferrier's  conclusions.  In  1899  Larionow  (52)  defined, 
also  from  experiments  on  cats,  remarkably  precise 
(though  incorrect)  boundaries  of  an  S-shaped  strip  of 
cortex  coursing  along  the  gyral  crest  beginning  at  the 
middle  ectosylvian  gyrus,  doubling  back  down  the 
posterior  ecto.sylvian  gyrus  and  redoubling  around  the 
inferior  end  of  the  posterior  suprasylvian  sulcus  for  a 
short  distance.  Larionow  was  the  first,  though  not  the 
last,  to  see  in  his  auditory  strip  a  representation  of  an 
'unfurled  cochlea',  an  expression  which  has  proved 
attractive  to  several  workers  through  the  years.  In- 
deed, though  Larionow  unrolled  his  cochlea  too  far 
back,  there  is  a  note  of  prophecy  and  a  modest  degree 
of  validity  to  the  concept,  as  later  events  have  shown. 

A  year  prior  to  Larionow's  report  Vogt  (104)  had 
pointed  otit  that  the  ectosylvian  cortex  of  the  carni- 
vore is  an  area  of  early  myelination.  By  the  turn  of 
the  century,  therefore,  the  feline  auditory  area  had 
been  located,  though  not  precisely  defined,  by  crude 
functional  methods;  the  same  area  had  been  shown 
to  have  special  histological  characteristics  and  the 
ideaof  cochlear  projection  had  been  introduced.  Thus, 
the  ideas  which  were  to  guide  the  future  study  of  the 
cortical  auditory  area  were  all  present.  The  subse- 
quent additions  can  be  thought  of  as  refinements  and 
variants  of  method,  the  advent  of  good  electrical 
recording  methods  during  the  1930's  constituting  the 
only  radical  departure  since.  Even  this  has  been  used 
without  much  change  in  pattern  of  thinking  until  very 
recently. 

There  are  several  ways  in  which  one  might  trace 
the  development  of  knowledge  of  the  auditory  cortex. 
In  order  to  show  how  we  have  arrived  at  our  present 
knowledge  and  attitudes  we  will  here  adopt  an  ap- 
proach which  will  be,  in  the  main,  sequential,  but 


will  deviate  from  strict  chronology  by  first  defining 
certain  questions  which  were  or  might  rea.sonably 
have  been  asked  at  the  beginning  and  considering  the 
successive  steps  which  have  been  taken  toward  an- 
swering these.  Thus,  departures  from  strict  chronology 
will  be  necessary  when  solutions  to  questions  have 
been  found  not  in  what  at  the  beginning  might  have 
tjecn  logical  sequence.  More  often  than  not  this  has 
occurred  when  it  was  generally  thought  that,  for 
example,  question  i  had  been  answered  and  one  might 
proceed  to  question  2,  only  to  find  in  the  cour.se  of 
investigation  of  question  2  that  question  i  had  not 
been  answered  as  fully  as  it  had  seemed. 

Given  the  general  location  of  a  functional  cortical 
area,  the  next  question  is  to  determine  the  extent  of 
the  area.  This  question  has  been  asked  at  least  tacitly 
in  nearly  all  investigations  for  over  50  years,  even 
when  the  stated  central  question  of  a  particular  study 
was  of  a  more  esoteric  nature.  One  reason  which 
makes  this  determination  an  almost  mandatory  start- 
ing point  for  any  study  of  the  auditory  cortex  is  that, 
because  of  individual  variation,  there  are  no  configur- 
ative  landmarks  which  can  be  relied  on  except  in  the 
most  general  way;  therefore,  if  the  experiment  pre- 
supposes exact  knowledge  of  extent  of  auditory  pro- 
jection, this  must  be  determined  for  each  animal  as 
the  starting  point. 

The  animal  most  frequently  used  in  experiments  on 
the  auditory  area  is  the  cat  with  the  monkey  (espe- 
cially macaque)  next  most  frequently;  the  dog  has 
been  used  in  only  a  few  cases.  Unless  otherwise 
specified,  the  ensuing  discussion  may  be  assumed  to 
refer  to  the  cat  as  the  experimental  animal.  Figure  2 
shows  the  standard  lateral  view  of  the  cat  brain  which 
will  be  used  in  subsequent  figures  in  portraying  the 
auditory  area  maps  of  several  studies. 

Vogt's  myelination  time  studies  represented  the 
first  application  of  a  detailed  morphological  method 
of  study  to  the  auditory  cortex.  Campbell  (21)  in  1905 
produced  the  first  careful  study  of  the  region  by  the 
cytoarchitectonic  method.  Campbell's  area  is  shown 
in  figure  3.  (In  this  figure,  the  total  extent  of  the 
cortex  considered  to  be  auditory  in  function  is  shown 
in  each  case.  Each  portrays  the  original  data  as 
nearly  as  these  could  be  projected  from  the  original 
publication  to  the  standard  view  of  the  cat  brain  used 
in  all.  Subdivisions  are  ignored  for  purposes  of  the 
immediate  discussion  but  will  be  considered  in  the 
next  section.)  It  is  interesting  to  note  that  Campbell's 
auditory  area  is  the  most  extensive  of  any  but  the 
latest  published  and  is  remarkably  similar  in  some 
respects  to  the  total  composite  area  which  would  ex- 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


393 


press   the   current   area   of  substantial   agreement   of 
several  authors. 

In  1937,  Kornmiiller  (50)  published  the  first  map 
of  the  cat's  auditory  cortex  determined  by  recording 
electrical  responses  to  acoustic  stimulation.  His  map, 
seen  in  figure  •],  includes  an  area  confined  to  middle 


FIG.  2.  Lateral  view  of  brain  of  cat.  FR,  rhinal  fissure;  GEA. 
anterior  ectosylvian  gyrus;  GEM,  middle  ectosylvian  gyrus, 
GEP,  posterior  ectosylvian  gyrus;  GPA,  anterior  pseudosylvian 
gyrus;  GPP,  posterior  pseudosylvian  gyrus;  GSA,  anterior 
suprasylvian  gyrus;  GSM,  middle  suprasylvian  gyrus;  GSP, 
posterior  suprasylvian  gyrus;  SEA,  anterior  ectosylvian  sulcus; 
SEP,  posterior  ectosylvian  sulcus;  SP,  pseudosylvian  sulcus; 
and  SSAI,  middle  suprasylvian  sulcus. 


ectosylvian  cortex.  Between  that  time  and  1941,  two 
other  such  maps  were  published.  Bremer  &  Dow  (17) 
and  Ades  (i),  using  brief  acoustic  stimuli  (clicks), 
defined  the  area  responsive  to  such  stimulation  largely 
in  the  middle  ectosylvian  gyrus  (fig.  4).  The  responsive 
area  of  Bremer  &  Dow  extends  to  the  pseudosylvian 
sulcus  while  that  of  Ades  stops  short  of  the  sulcus. 
Bremer  &  Dow  studied  the  cytoarchitectonic  char- 
acteristics of  the  region  also  and  found  the  somewhat 
smaller  area  shown  in  the  same  figure  to  satisfy  the 
criteria  of  a  sensory  projection  area.  The  study  of 
WooUard  &  Harpman  (iio),  in  which  they  traced 
Marchi  degeneration  after  electrolytic  lesions  in  the 
media!  geniculate  body,  defined  the  area  shown  in 
figure  4  as  the  projection  area  of  that  nucleus.  It  is 
interesting  to  note  that  the  Woollard  &  Harpman 
map,  based  on  anatomical  findings,  corresponds  more 
closely  with  the  Bremer  &  Dow  electrical  response 
map  than  the  latter  does  with  their  own  cytoarchi- 
tectonic map,  which  itself  more  nearly  coincides  with 
Ades'  electrical  response  map. 

The  maps  of  the  feline  auditory  cortex  derived  from 
the  foregoing  studies  from  1933  to  1941  share  the 
common  feature  of  being  considerably  more  restricted 
than  the  much  earlier  work  of  Campbell  indicated. 
They  were  at  the  time  regarded  as  being  reasonably 
consistent  with  each  other  and  probably  substantially 
valid  in  defining  the  "  primary'  auditory  projection 
area,  the  only  point  of  disagreement  being  the  exten 


WOOLLARD 
AND    HARPMAN 


1939 


VOGT    1898 


KORNMULLER     1933 


BREMER  AND  DOW     1939 


ADES     1941 


FIG.  3  (top).  Auditory  area  of  cat  as  described  by  individuals  named,  shown  by  shaded  areas. 
All  redrawn  from  originals  on  standard  view. 

FIG.  4  (bottom).  Auditory  area  of  cat  as  described  by  individuals  named,  shown  by  shaded 
areas.  All  redrawn  from  originals  on  standard  view.  In  the  map  of  Bremer  &  Dow,  electrical  re- 
sponses to  clicks  could  be  obtained  over  both  the  horizonlalh  and  verlicatly  shaded  arcaj ;  cytoarchi- 
tectonically,  the  vertically  shaded  area  satisfied  the  criteria  for  a  sensory  projection  area. 


594 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


of  the  lateral  (ventral)  margin  of  the  area.  The  pos- 
terior ectosylvian  cortex  is  the  one  part  of  the  area 
that  none  of  these  studies  implicated  as  auditory  in 
function.  This  is  a  particularly  curious  circumstance 
in  the  light  of  evidence  which  began  to  accumulate 
rapidly  the  following  year  (1942)  and  which  demon- 
strated that  the  posterior  ectosylvian  cortex  is  most 
definitely  auditory. 

Two  virtually  concurrent  physiological  studies  ex- 
tended the  cortical  sphere  of  auditory  response  to  the 
posterior  ectosylvian  gyrus  in  1942  and  1943.  Both 
represented  a  departure  from  the  preceding  studies  in 
that  functional  subdivision  or  organization  rather  than 
total  extent  of  the  auditory  cortex  became  the  princi- 
pal theme  although  extension  of  the  boundaries  also 
came  as  a  by-product.  Both  these  studies  and  later 
ones  which  grew  out  of  them  demonstrated  how  much 
the  factor  of  adequate  instrumentation  may  influence 
validity  of  data.  Ades  (2)  demonstrated  what  was  then 
termed  a 'secondary'  auditory  area  (see  fig.  5)  occupy- 
ing most  of  the  posterior  ectosylvian  cortex.  The 
experiments  consisted  in  mapping  the  area  responsive 
to  clicks,  then  applying  strychnine  to  the  '  primary' 
area  .so  defined  and  remapping  the  responsive  area 
which  now  included  the  posterior  ectosylvian.  The 
latter  area  was  originally  termed  'secondary'  because 
its  response  appeared  to  be  dependent  on  and  driven 
by  that  of  the  'primary'  middle  ectosylvian  cortex. 
This  terminology  was  further  motivated  by  a  preoc- 
cupation, dating  from  Campbell's  time,  with  the 
concept  of  primary  sensory  projection  areas  sur- 
rounded by  or  adjacent  to  sensory 'association'  areas. 
Repetition  of  the  same  experiments  with  more  nearly 
adequate  instruments  (14,  48,  49)  has  demonstrated 
that  while  the  posterior  ectosylvian,  under  the  influ- 
ence of  strychnine,  is  driven  by  the  middle  ectosylvian, 
its  response  is  not  wholly  dependent  on  transmission 
through  the  middle  ectosylvian. 


Wool.sey  &  VValzl  (113)  published  a  report  (actually 
a  few  months  earlier  than  the  one  by  Ades,  though 
imknown  to  the  latter  until  after  his  own  report  was 
in  process  of  publication)  which  also  extended  the 
auditory  area  to  the  posterior  ectosylvian  cortex  and 
also  provided  a  basis  for  .subdivision  of  the  total 
responsive  area  but  on  a  quite  different  basis  than 
that  suggested  by  the  strychnine  experiments.  The 
experiments  reported  in  this  paper  by  Woolsey  & 
Walzl  are  worthy  of  special  note,  as  they  represent  a 
turning  point  in  research  on  the  auditory  cortex  which 
provides  the  basis  for  the  modern  view  point.  They 
employed  a  more  adequate  system  of  amplification 
and  recording  than  had  previously  been  used.  This, 
together  with  stimulation  of  small  groups  of  nerve 
fibers  in  the  exposed  osseous  spiral  lamina  of  the 
cochlea,  afforded  by  far  the  most  precise  technique 
yet  brought  to  bear  on  the  problem.  In  addition,  the 
results  had  great  influence  in  dispelling  the  bemuse- 
mcnt  with  the  concept  of  primary  and  secondary 
areas,  which,  while  it  may  still  have  some  degree  of 
validity,  was  in  retrospect  a  concept  which  had  done 
little  to  advance,  and  possibly  something  to  retard, 
the  development  of  understanding  of  cortical  auditory 
function. 

Woolsey  &  VValzl  stimulated  electrically  small  local 
groups  of  the  exposed  ends  of  cochlear  nerve  fibers  in 
the  osseous  spiral  lamina  and  recorded  the  cortical 
response.  They  were  able  to  show  that  local  stimula- 
tion of  such  small  groups  of  fibers  elicited  a  similarly 
localized  response  within  the  ectosylvian  cortex.  The 
pattern  of  projection  was  an  orderly  one  such  that  in 
the  more  superiorly  lying  strip  (fig.  5)  stimulation  at 
the  base  of  the  cochlea  evoked  response  anteriorly 
while  stimulation  of  the  apex  produced  it  posteriorly. 
In  the  more  laterally  lying  strip,  the  pattern  is  re- 
versed so  that  the  base  of  the  cochlea  projects  pos- 
teriorly and  the  apex  anteriorly.  These  results  were 


ADES    1943 


WOOLSEY 

AND    WALZL  1942 


ROSE 
AND    WOOLSEY   1949 


FIG.  5.  .'\uditory  area  of  cat  as  described  by  individuals  named.  .\\l  redrawn  from  originals  on 
standard  view.  Ades:  Vertical  shading,  'primary  area';  horizontal,  secondary  area.'  Woolsey  &  Walzl: 
Horizontal  shading,  A  I;  vertical,  A  II.  Rose  &  Woolsey:  Horizontal  shading,  EP;  vertical,  A  II;  cross- 
hatched,  A  I. 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


595 


confirmed  by  experiments  reported  by  the  same  au- 
thors (io8)  in  which  the  deficits  in  cortical  response 
to  click  stimulation  were  noted  after  local  lesions  in 
the  cochlea.  These  experiments  then  introduce  in  the 
auditory  cortex  an  organization  based  on  an  internal 
integrity  of  the  auditory  projection  pathway  such  that 
the  anatomical  pattern  of  the  cochlea  seems  to  be 
faithfully  represented  in  the  cortical  receiving  station. 
A  fuller  discussion  of  this  'point-to-point'  feature  of 
anatomical  projection  and  its  functional  implications 
will  be  found  in  another  section  of  this  chapter. 

Woolsey  &  Walzl  introduced  the  terminology  by 
which  the  two  strips  of  auditory  cortex  noted  in  the 
foregoing  paragraph  were  designated  respectively 
'A  r  (the  superior  or  dorsal  strip)  and  'A  IF  (the 
inferior  or  lateral  strip).  When  this  organization  of 
the  cortical  auditory  field  was  subjected  to  further 
study  involving,  in  addition  to  electrophysiological 
methods,  cytoarchitecture  and  retrograde  degenera- 
tion in  the  medial  geniculate  body  following  selective 
extirpation  of  parts  of  the  auditory  cortex  by  Rose 
(83)  and  Rose  &  Woolsey  (85),  a  further  revision  of 
the  terminology  became  necessary.  For,  as  their  re- 
sults showed,  A  I  occupies  a  more  limited  area  than 
originally  designated  by  Woolsey  &  Walzl  (fig.  5)  and 
is  the  only  part  whose  destruction  leads  to  widespread 
degeneration  in  the  pars  principalis  of  the  medial 
geniculate.  Cytoarchitectonic  study  shows  that  A  I 
and  A  II  differ  from  each  other  and  the  anterior  parts 
of  both  diflPer  from  the  posterior.  These  findings  lead 
to  the  map  presented  by  Rose  &  Woolsey  (fig.  5)  in 
which  the  auditory  area  is  now  divided  into  A  I,  A 
II,  and  EP,  the  latter  being  compounded  of  the  pos- 
terior parts  of  the  original  A  I  and  A  II.  It  will  be 
noted  that  these  areas  now  show  varying  degrees  of 
correspondence  to  those  of  earlier  studies.  For  ex- 
ample, A  I  now  is  closely  similar  to  the  more  restricted 
electrically  responsive  area  shown  by  Ades  (i)  and 
Kormiiller  (50)  and  to  the  cytoarchitectonic  maps  of 
Bremer  &  Dow  (17)  and  Waller  (106).  It  also  cor- 
responds to  the  posteroinferior  portion  of  Vogt's  (104) 
old  map  based  on  myelination  time.  A  I  plus  A  II 
now  resembles  the  electrical  map  area  of  Bremer  & 
Dow  (17),  the  geniculocortical  projection  area  of 
Woollard  &  Harpman  (iio),  and  corresponds  with 
somewhat  lesser  fidelity  to  the  anterior  part  of  the 
Campbell  (21)  map.  EP  corresponds  clo.sely  to  the 
posterior  ectosylvian  'secondary  area'  of  Ades  (2).  It 
would  appear  that  the  restriction  of  responsive  area 
shown  in  the  earlier  o,scilloscopic  studies  may  have 
been  due  to  the  relative  weakness  of  responses  in  A  II 
(except  at  its  anterior  end)  and  in  EP  which  were 


not  detected  by  the  comparatively  poor  instruments 
then  available.  The  re-emergence  of  the  EP  area, 
as  it  is  now  commonly  called,  plus  the  reaffirmation 
by  Kiang  (49)  that  EP  is  to  .some  extent  functionally 
dominated  by  A  I  revives  the  question  of  the  func- 
tional significance  of  such  a  cortical  interrelationship. 

At  this  point,  while  it  has  become  apparent  that 
the  limits  of  the  cortex  which  can  be  activated  by 
acoustic  stimulation  may  not  have  been  completely 
and  finally  defined,  it  will  be  useful  to  depart  briefly 
from  the  development  of  this  essentially  anatomical 
concept  to  consider  some  functional  studies.  These 
are  of  interest  not  only  as  they  contribute  to  correla- 
tion of  structure  and  function,  but  also  as  they  reflect 
on  the  extent  and  internal  organization  of  the  auditory 
cortex. 

The  history  of  functional  studies  of  the  auditory 
cortex  is,  to  a  great  extent,  a  history  of  increasing 
complexity  of  stimulus  and  experimental  learning 
situations.  It  is  also  a  study  in  progression  of  con- 
ceptualization of  auditory  function.  It  begins  with  the 
experimenter  striving  for  valid  criteria  to  show  simply 
whether  or  not  the  experimental  animal  hears  and 
continues  at  present  as  a  search  for  ways  in  which 
auditory  discriminative  ability  of  animals  can  be 
accurately  assessed. 

Some  of  the  earlier  efforts  to  estimate  the  cortical 
contribution  to  hearing  in  animals  took  the  form  of 
hearing  tests  of  greater  or  lesser  refinement,  following 
total  or  hemidecortication  (12,  38).  It  was  demon- 
strated that  the  decorticate  dog  can  still  acquire  a 
crude  conditioned  response  to  sound  although  not 
nearly  as  readily  as  an  intact  animal.  Although  the 
animal  could  acquire  the  habit,  his  absolute  intensity 
threshold  was  higher  by  70  db  (38).  Other  workers 
(12)  were  less  impressed  by  the  auditory  deficit  in 
decorticate  cats.  The  decorticate  animal,  however, 
shows  a  general  debility  and  inattentiveness  which  is 
more  impressive  than  an  auditory  or  any  other  specific 
sensory  defect.  This  leads  one  to  suspect  that  any  test 
of  hearing  in  such  a  preparation  may  be  contaminated 
to  a  considerable  degree  by  other  deficits  which  have 
more  to  do  with  general  integrative  capacity  than 
with  hearing  per  se. 

To  avoid  this  difficulty,  several  workers  resorted 
to  extirpations  of,  as  they  thought,  specifically  audi- 
tory cortex.  The  theory  was  that  if  the  cortical  audi- 
tory projection  area  is  removed,  then  the  entire 
cerebral  cortex  is  effectively  eliminated  from  partici- 
pation in  any  learning  or  conditioning  process  that 
involves  stimulation  by  sound.  If  this  were  so,  then 
any  auditory  function  present  before  but  absent  after 


396 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


operation  couW  be  said  to  depend  upon  mediation 
through  the  auditory  cortex.  (Of  course,  as  we  now 
know,  the  geniculotemporal  radiation  is  not  quite  the 
bottleneck  it  was  then  supposed.  It  is  not  the  only 
avenue  through  which  excitation  aroused  by  sound 
may  reach  the  cerebral  cortex,  and  it  may  not  ije 
the  only  effective  avenue  by  which  such  excitation 
can  produce  a  specifically  auditory  cortical  sign.)  A 
series  of  studies  in  which  various  aspects  of  hearing 
were  tested  before  and  after  auditory  cortical  extirpa- 
tion were  carried  out  beginning  about  20  years  ago. 
These  yielded  results  which  were  surprising  because 
of  the  difficulty  encountered  in  seriously  impairing 
auditory  function.  Several  specific  studies  are  con- 
sidered in  the  following  paragraph. 

The  earlier  workers  in  this  area  were  convinced 
that  the  intensity  threshold  of  hearing  for  pure  tones 
constituted  the  proper  initial  criterion  of  cortical 
auditory  function.  This  led  immediateh  to  apparent 
discrepancies  in  results  between  different  laboratories 
and  even  different  individuals  in  the  same  laboratories 
(51,  58,  74)  because,  as  ultimately  became  clear,  the 
factor  of  recovery  time  between  extirpation  of  the 
auditory  cortex  and  retesting  of  thresholds  is  crucial. 
Those  who  were  retested  within  a  very  short  time 
showed  varying  degrees  of  threshold  elevation,  those 
who  waited  several  days  or  weeks  before  being  re- 
tested  demonstrated  little  or  no  loss  of  acuity  for  pure 
tones.  Finally  Girden  (37)  demonstrated  that  in  the 
dog,  after  incomplete  lesions  of  the  auditory  cortex, 
initial  losses  in  acuity  gave  way  with  continued  testing 
and  the  thresholds  returned  nearly  to  preoperative 
levels.  Even  then,  the  blame  for  discrepant  results 
tended  to  be  fixed  on  differences  in  testing  methods 
and  on  the  degree  of  completeness  of  destruction  of 
auditory  cortex,  the  latter  factor  being  complicated 
further  by  differences  in  understanding  of  extent  of 
auditory  cortex  and  by  this  kind  of  experiment  it.self 
being  used  as  a  criterion  of  determining  that  extent. 
Kryter  &  Ades  (51)  demonstrated  that  ab.solutc  in- 
tensity threshold  to  pure  tones  does  not  rise  appreci- 
ably due  to  extirpation  of  auditory  cortex  in  the  cat, 
even  when  the  cortical  lesions  in  some  instances 
extended  considerably  beyond  the  widest  boundaries 
suggested  for  the  area.  By  this  time,  workers  were 
despairing  of  the  intensity  threshold  to  pure  tone  as  a 
reliable  indicator  of  cortical  auditory  function.  It  be- 
came apparent,  in  retrospect,  that  the  confusion  of 
previous  studies  had  occurred,  at  least  in  part,  becau.se 
simple  acuity  as  measured  in  this  way  is  simply  not 
dependent  on  cortical  participation.  It  appeared  logi- 
cal then  to  .seek  .some  more  complex  manifestation  of 


auditory  function  which  could  be  tested  by  a  condi- 
tioning method  and  which  might  prove  to  be  de- 
pendent on  auditory  cortex. 

A  series  of  studies  directed  toward  that  end  began 
in  1946  with  the  report  of  Raab  &  Ades  (74)  indicat- 
ing that,  while  of  interest  in  other  respects,  the  func- 
tion of  discrimination  of  differences  in  intensity  of 
sound,  measured  in  terms  of  difference  limens,  was 
not  the  cortex-bound  function  .sought.  This  impression 
was  confirmed  by  Rosenzweig  (87).  The  next  obvious 
point  of  attack  was  the  ability  of  the  animal  to 
discriminate  between  small  differences  in  frequency 
before  and  after  extirpation  of  auditory  areas.  This 
kind  of  study  has  been  done  by  Butler  et  al.  (20),  by 
Meyer  &  Woolsey  (64)  and  by  Allen  (9).  Before 
discussing  these  studies,  it  is  necessary  to  digress 
briefly  to  note  the  addition  of  still  another  cortical 
area  which  shows  .signs  ol  auditory  function. 

In  1945,  Tunturi  (103)  described  in  the  dog  an 
area  in  which  electrical  response  to  auditory  stimula- 
tion could  be  evoked.  This  area  lies  outside  any  of 
those  previously  described  as  auditory  in  the  dog  or 
as  homologous  areas  in  the  cat.  It  lies  in  fact  partly 
in  the  second  somatic  area  (43).  Also  in  1945  Allen 
(9)  using  Tunturi's  map  found  that,  whereas  aljlation 
of  the  traditional  auditory  areas  temporarily  impaired 
but  failed  to  destroy  permanently  the  ability  of  dogs 
to  discriminate  widely  different  frequencies,  this 
ability  was  permanently  lost  if  the  third  auditorv  area 
of  Timturi  were  also  destroyed.  Later  studies  on  the 
cat  have  confirmed  the  fact  that  auditory  stimulation 
elicits  response  in  the  second  somatic  area  (15,   16, 

65,  70)- 

Meyer  &  Woolsey  (64)  trained  cats  to  respond  to 
change  in  frequency  of  a  gi\en  tone  at  irregularly 
spaced  intervals  in  a  series  of  2  sec.  tones  which  were 
otherwise  alike.  Once  the  cats  were  trained,  a  rough 
difference  limen  for  frequency  was  determined.  They 
then  extirpated,  symmetrically,  in  \arying  combina- 
tion the  following  cortical  areas:  A  I,  A  II,  EP, 
suprasylvian  gyrus,  temporal  region  (see  fig.  6)  and 
the  cerebellar  tuber  vermis.  Following  operation,  the 
animals  were  retrained  and  retested.  It  was  foimd 
that  if  A  I,  A  II,  EP  and  S  II  (second  somatic  area) 
were  completely  destroyed  on  both  sides,  the  animals 
could  no  longer  achie\e  the  frequenc\-  discrimination. 
No  other  combination  of  lesions  had  this  efl'cct  and 
if  remnants  of  A  I  and  A  II  escaped  damage,  fre- 
quency discrimination  was  maintained.  Butler  el  al. 
(20)  used  a  basically  similar  plan  but  with  what  they 
felt  was  a  more  reliable  and  critical  method  of  testing. 
In  addition,   they  carefulK'  analyzed  the  retrograde 


CENTRAL    AUDIT(JRV    MECHANISMS 


597 


FIG.  6.  Composite  view  of  all  areas  of  cat  brain  showing 
auditory  function.  A  I,  first  auditory  area;  A  II,  second  auditory 
area;  EP,  posterior  ectosylvian  area;  S  II,  second  somatic  area; 
IN,  insular  region;  TE,  temporal  area. 


thalamic  degeneration  in  their  cat.s.  The  rcsuhs  in 
this  series  of  experiments  differ  from  those  of  Meyer 
&  Woolsey  in  that  ability  to  discriminate  frequency 
was  not  permanently  impaired  even  after  complete 
lesions  of  A  I,  A  II,  EP  and  S  II.  Three  significant 
points  in  explanation  of  the  apparent  discrepancy 
were  offered  by  Butler  et  al.  /)  The  testing  methods, 
as  have  already  been  mentioned  above  were  different, 
r)  In  the  Meyer-Woolsey  animals  with  loss  of  dis- 
crimination, the  lesions,  though  listed  as  including 
A  I,  A  II,  EP  and  S  II,  actually  extended  ventrally 
nearly  to  the  rhinal  fissure  (unlike  those  of  Butler  et 
al.^.  j)  In  the  latter  group,  the  posterior  part  of  the 
medial  geniculate,  pars  principalis,  consistently  es- 
caped degeneration,  although  it  was  also  noted  that 
the  nearer  to  the  rhinal  fissure  the  lesion  approached, 
the  farther  posterior  crept  the  degeneration  in  the 
medial  geniculate.  Thus,  the  tissue  lying  before  and 
behind  the  p.seudosylvian  sulcus,  hitherto  largely  im- 
mune to  implication  in  the  auditory  cortical  sphere, 
began  to  take  on  a  most  suspiciously  acoustic  flavor. 
That  this  trend  is  essentially  correct  has  been  demon- 
strated in  recent  experiments  by  Neff  and  his  group 
and  in  the  recent  critical  analysis  by  Rose  &  Woolsey 
(86)  of  thalamic  degeneration  resulting  from  lesions 
of  the  several  subdivisions  of  auditory  and  apparently 
related  cortex  singly  and  in  combinations. 

Diamond  &  Neff  (24)  trained  cats  to  respond  to 
change  in  a  simple  tonal  pattern.  A  three-tone  se- 
quence, for  example,  of  low-high-low  was  presented 
repetitively  for  a  variable  number  of  tiines  and  then 
changed  abruptly  to  high-low-high,  at  which  point 


the  animal,  in  the  course  of  trairiin^,  learned  to 
respond  (by  moving  across  the  middje  of  a  shuttle 
bo.x)  to  avoid  shock.  Extirpation  qf  A  I  failed  to 
disturb  the  habit  of  discriminating  tht;  two  patterns. 
With  extensive  damage  to  A  II  and  EF  in  addition  to 
-A  I,  the  habit  wa";  temporarily  lost  but  could  be  re- 
established by  further  training.  If  the  destruction  of 
all  three  areas  was  complete,  the  tonal  pattern  dis- 
crimination could  not  be  re-established  even  with  a 
prolonged  period  of  retraining.  It  is  interesting  to 
note  that  even  small  remnants  of  tissue  which  could 
be  excited  by  sound,  and  which  closely  adjoined 
ablated  areas,  were  sufiicient  to  make  possible  retrain- 
ing of  the  tonal  pattern  discrimination.  In  a  second 
series  of  experiments,  Goldberg  et  al.  (39),  having 
trained  cats  to  both  a  simple  frequency  discrimination 
habit  and  to  the  tonal  pattern  discrimination,  now 
extirpated  bilaterally  the  region  ventral  to  A  II  and 
EP  (insular  and  temporal  cortex  shown  in  fig.  6), 
sparing  A  I,  A  II  and  EP  as  demonstrated  by  subse- 
quent responsiveness  to  click  stimulation  and  absence 
of  severe  degeneration  in  the  medial  geniculate.  The 
results  were  quite  surprising,  both  simple  tone  dis- 
crimination and  tonal  pattern  discrimination  being 
lost  after  operation.  It  proved  possible  to  re-establish 
simple  discrimination  in  about  the  same  time  as  that 
required  for  original  training.  On  the  contrary,  pat- 
tern discrimination  could  not  be  relearned  even  with 
prolonged  retraining.  The  behavior  of  the  animals  in 
the  test  situation  was  not  visibly  different  from  pre- 
operative behavior  and,  since  the  frequency  discrimi- 
nation habit  was  relearned,  one  cannot  attribute  the 
results  to  loss  of  learning  capacity;  rather  the  loss  of 
pattern  discrimination  seems  to  be  a  specific  auditory 
deficit. 

Two  salient  features,  then,  emerge  from  the  recent 
work  of  Neff  and  his  group.  /)  The  insular  and  tem- 
poral cortex  of  the  cat  are  demonstrated  to  be  of 
crucial  importance  to  at  least  some  aspects  of  auditory 
integrative  function.  2)  Discrimination  of  tonal  pat- 
terns (as  distinguished  from  simple  change  in  fre- 
quency) appears  to  be  cortically  bound. 

One  cannot  help  recalling  (at  least  this  author 
cannot)  experiments  in  x'isual  discrimination  (4,  7)  in 
which  somewhat  similar  results  were  obtained  after 
lesions  of  areas  18  and  19  and  the  temporal  lobe  in 
the  monkey.  It  suijsequently  proved,  however,  that 
losses  of  discriminative  ability  in  the  monkey  were  less 
permanent  if  the  monkey  had  been  trained  to  learn 
quickly  many  different  visually-guided  discrimina- 
tions rather  than  just  one  (81,  82).  Although  the 
evidence  is  insufficient,  one  cannot  help  but  wonder 


598 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


if  the  insular  and  temporal  cortex  of  the  cat  ha\e  a 
significance  to  the  cat's  auditory  function  similar  to 
that  of  areas  i8  and  ig  and  the  temporal  cortex  to 
the  monkey's  visual  function.  Also,  though  prophecy 
is  a  questionable  if  not  dangerous  indulgence  for 
ordinary  people,  one  also  wonders,  given  an  animal 
whose  sophistication  is  augmented  by  the  learning  of 
many  rather  than  a  single  type  of  auditory  discrimina- 
tion, whether  the  particular  habit  (tonal  pattern  in 
this  case)  would  remain  as  firmly  corticalizcd. 

Rose  &  Woolsey  (85)  introduce  in  their  study  an 
anatomical  concept  which  goes  a  long  way  toward 
clearing  up  at  least  one  aspect  of  the  interrelationship 
between  the  subdivisions  of  the  cat's  auditory  cortex 
and  also  toward  providing  anatomical  support  for  the 
work  of  the  Neff  group.  This  is  based  on  the  shifting 
pattern  of  degeneration  observed  in  the  medial  genic- 
ulate body  as  the  cortical  lesions  are  varied  in  pattern 
to  include  one  or  more  of  the  subdivisions  (fig.  6 
indicates  the  areas  and  the  terminology  applied  to 
them).  It  is  considered  that  a  cortical  area  receives  an 
essential  projection  from  a  given  thalamic  nucleus  if 
destruction  of  that  area  and  only  that  area  results  in 
marked  degeneration  in  the  thalamic  nucleus.  If,  how- 
ever, two  cortical  areas  are  considered  and  if  destruc- 
tion of  neither  of  these  alone  causes  degeneration  in  a 
given  thalamic  nucleus,  but  simultaneous  destruction 
of  both  does  lead  to  severe  degeneration  in  that 
nucleus,  both  cortical  areas  are  said  to  receive  sustain- 
ing projections  from  the  nucleus.  On  this  basis,  Rose 
&  Woolsey  found  that  the  only  component  of  the 
cortical  auditory  complex  which  receives  an  essential 
projection  from  the  medial  geniculate  (pars  princi- 
palis) is  A  I.  Since,  however,  simultaneous  destruction 
of  A  I,  A  II,  and  EP  result  in  much  more  profound 
medial  geniculate  degeneration  than  does  A  I  alone, 
it  is  considered  that  both  All  and  EP  receive  sustain- 
ing projections  from  the  geniculate  pars  principalis. 
Even  the  combination  leaves  the  posterior  third  of  the 
nucleus  relatively  unscathed.  It  is  only  when  the 
cortical  destruction  is  extended  ventrally  to  include 
all  of  the  cortex  between  A  II  and  EP  and  the  rhinal 
fissure  (temporal  and  insular  cortex  as  shown  in  fig.  6) 
that  severe  degeneration  extends  posteriorly  to  include 
the  entire  pars  principalis.  There  is,  therefore,  projec- 
tion from  the  posterior  sector  of  the  pars  principalis 
to  the  temporal  and  insular  cortex.  That  this  is 
probably  a  sustaining  projection  is  attested  by  the 
fact  that  the  still  limited  evidence  indicates  insular 
and  temporal  lesions  alone  fail  to  produce  severe 
posterior  sector  degeneration.  Similarly,  the  pars 
magnocellularis  degenerates  markedly  only  when  A  I, 


A  II,  EP,  insular  and  temporal  area'*  are  all  destroyed. 
Both  posterior  pars  principalis  and  magnocellularis 
are  largely  preserved  by  the  preservation  of  A  I  alone; 
consequently,  it  would  appear  that  both  emit  rather 
widespread  sustaining  projections,  but  there  is  no 
evidence  as  yet  of  emission  of  essential  projections. 
Finally,  from  the  limited  available  evidence,  the 
anterior  part  of  the  posterior  nuclear  group  of  the 
thalamus,  in  addition  to  the  medial  geniculate,  must 
be  suspected  of  having  auditory  connections.  The 
critical  evidence  is  lacking  but  this  thalamic  area, 
lying  between  auditory  and  tactile  nuclei,  probably 
sends  a  sustaining  projection  to  S  II,  which  itself  has 
ijeen  shown  to  be  excitable  by  auditory  stimulation 
(15,  16,  19,  65,  70).  Moreover,  this  auditory  excita- 
bility, according  to  Rose  &  Woolsey  (85),  .seems  to  be 
independent  of  medial  geniculate-auditory  cortex 
activity  since  it  appears  even  when  the  medial  genic- 
ulate body  is  profoundly  degenerated.  In  contrast  to 
this  conclusion,  Priljram  ft  al.  (73),  noting  an  ap- 
parently similar  system  in  the  monkey,  maintain  that 
the  responses  in  S  II  do  drop  out  upon  degeneration 
of  the  medial  geniculate,  and  so  come  to  the  conclu- 
sion that  the  interconnection  is  by  way  of  collaterals 
from  the  medial  geniculate.  Until  this  conflict  is  re- 
solved, therefore,  the  question  of  the  essential  connec- 
tion of  this  thalamic  nucleus  and  cortical  area  with 
the  cochlear  projection  pathway  must  be  left  open. 

Returning  briefly  to  efforts  toward  finding  some 
auditory  integrative  function  which  is  corticalizcd  in 
the  sense  that  the  task  cannot  be  accomplished  with- 
out cortical  participation,  the  work  of  Neff  f <  al.  (67) 
deserves  special  attention.  They  trained  cats  to  make  a 
response  requiring  localization  of  sound  in  space,  cor- 
rect performance  being  rewarded  with  food.  Lesions 
were  then  made  in  A  I  in  .some  cases  and  A  I,  A  II  and 
EP  in  others.  Bilateral  destruction  even  of  A  I,  if 
complete,  caused  severe  deterioration  of  performance 
in  the  test  situation.  That  this  behavioral  deficit  was 
specific  to  hearing  was  demonstrated  by  a  normal 
capacity  to  learn  a  problem  in  the  same  situation  if  it 
were  based  on  visual  cues.  As  the  authors  point  out, 
while  the  auditory  cortex  must  play  an  important  role 
in  the  function  of  localization  of  sound  in  space,  it  is 
less  clear  what  the  nature  of  this  role  may  be.  The 
evidence  would  allow  several  hypotheses  but  select 
none  of  them.  The  authors  list  the  following.  /)  Intact 
auditory  cortex  is  essential  to  learning  the  relationship 
between  auditory  signal  and  food  reward.  2)  Intact 
auditory  cortex  is  essential  for  maintaining  attention 
to  auditory  signal,  attention  being  defined  as  the 
abilitv  of  the  animal  to  orient  toward  the  signal  and 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


599 


carry  through  its  appropriately  directed  activity  until 
the  full  response  of  opening  a  door  and  obtaining;  food 
is  accomplished,  jj)  Intact  auditory  cortex  is  necessary 
for  accurate  localization  of  sound  in  space.  As  indi- 
cated above,  the  data  fail  clearly  to  single  out  any  of 
these.  This,  incidentally,  is  a  common  finding  in 
behavioral  experiments  involving  extirpation  ot  brain 
tissue.  It  may  be  an  inherent  failing  in  all  such  ex- 
periments. However,  this  is  of  more  concern  from  the 
standpoint  of  the  neurology  of  learning  than  from  that 
of  specifically  auditory  integration. 

We  cannot  leave  the  subject  of  definition  of  the 
auditory  area  without  referring  to  the  work  of 
Lilly  (55,  56)  who  has  introduced  a  new  method 
and  a  new  dimension  to  this  field  of  study.  Using  a 
square  array  of  25  electrodes  covering  an  area  of 
cortex  of  i  cm'-,  25  amplifiers  and  glow  tubes,  each 
channel  serving  one  electrode,  and  photographing 
at  128  frames  per  sec.  the  bank  of  glow  tubes,  Lilly 
has  been  able  to  demonstrate  the  patterns  of  spon- 
tanous  electrical  activity  and  those  evoked  by  acous- 
tic stimulation  in  the  cat's  auditory  cortex.  In  this 
fashion,  the  simultaneous  cortical  surface  activity 
can  be  recorded  at  25  different  zones  and  the  changes 
at  each  noted  in  time  sequence.  Thus  Lilly  has 
demonstrated  what  he  calls  'forms  and  figures'  of 
cortical  activity  which  combine  the  dimensions  of 
time  and  space  in  a  way  not  previously  possible. 
The  array  was  placed  across  the  upper  end  of  the 
posterior  ectosylvian  sulcus  so  that  it  covered  part 
of  the  junctional  area  of  A  I  and  A  II  with  EP. 
Lilly  found  that  both  spontaneous  activity  and  the 
response  to  clicks  followed  a  definite,  repeatable 
pattern.  Under  deep  anesthesia  the  response  to  clicks 
would  appear  first  in  one  corner  of  the  array,  spread 
posteriorly  to  a  boundary  and  there  die  out.  Pos- 
terior to  the  boundary  (i.e.  in  EP)  spontaneous 
waves  tended  to  originate  and  travel  downwards 
along  the  posterior  ectosylvian  gyrus;  however, 
under  lighter  anesthesia  the  response  wave  could 
trigger  the  'spontaneous'  EP  waves,  and  at  still 
lighter  levels  spontaneous  waves  were  seen  to  origi- 
nate in  A  I  and  travel  across  the  border.  In  another 
series  of  experiments  on  unanesthetized  monkeys, 
clicks  set  ofT  waves  of  activity  which  were  observed 
to  travel  systematically  over  the  sensorimotor  cortex. 
Thus,  Lilly  has  at  once  made  several  interesting 
points,  some  of  which  are  of  specific  interest  in  the 
development  of  knowledge  of  the  cortical  auditory 
equipment  and  others  are  of  even  greater  significance 
to  neurophysiological  thinking  in  general.  With 
respect  to  auditory  function,  he  has  shown  that  there 


is  a  certain  validity  to  the  accepted  subdivision  of 
auditory  cortex,  albeit  this  may  have  been  to  some 
degree  overplayed  in  the  past  because  most  previous 
workers  (with  one  qualified  exception  in  Bremer') 
have  used  deeply  anesthetized  animals  as  the  stand- 
ard preparation.  At  the  least,  these  studies  present 
the  interrelationship  of  auditory  subdivisions  from  a 
new  \iewpoint.  He  has  further  demonstrated  that 
the  excitation  of  cerebral  cortex  which  results  from 
acoustic  stimulation  may  be  (or  perhaps  always  is) 
considerably  more  widespread  than  is  usually  as- 
sumed, tacitly  at  least,  in  the  plan  of  auditory  ex- 
periments. This  is  somewhat  disquieting  from  the 
standpoint  of  planning  an  experiment  to  demon- 
strate by  electrophysiological  method  some  facet  of 
cortical  auditory  function;  howe\er,  it  is  perhaps 
potentially  comforting  in  even  greater  degree  to 
those  who  work  with  behavioral  methods  and  are 
constantly  confronted  with  the  necessity  of  explain- 
ing why  an  animal  in  which  access  to  cortical  in- 
tegrative processes  has  presumably  been  denied  to 
the  acoustic  system  (by  remoxal  of  receptive  areas) 
can  yet  behave  as  though  auditory  stimulation  still 
held  meaning  for  him. 

From  a  more  general  \iewpoint,  Lilly  has  neatly 
demonstrated  the  restrictive  elTect  of  anesthetization 
on  cortical  activity  with  respect  to  both  time  and 
space  such  that  the  functional  separation  of  con- 
tiguous areas  tends  to  be  exaggerated.  It  should  be 
an  ample  indication  that  while  unguarded  use  of 
anesthesia  in  electrophysiological  studies  of  the 
cortex  may  relieve  some  technical  problems  for  the 
experimenter,  it  may  simultaneously  furnish  the 
basis  for  an  abundance  of  conceptual  'red  herrings.' 
Lilly's  work  suggests  further,  however,  that  the  en- 
lightened, controlled  use  of  anesthesia  may  be  of 
most  positive  value  in  cortical  studies  by  virtue  of 
its  capacity  to  separate  functional  areas  whose 
boundaries  tend  to  be  inconspicuous  in  the  waking 
animal. 

The  reader  will  note  that,  having  begun  with  a 
hazy  idea  of  the  location  and  limits  of  the  auditory 
cortex  of  the  cat,  these  gradually  became  sharply 
defined  through  the  years  with  improvement  in 
instrumentation,  method  and  thinking.  At  several 
points  in  this  history,  the  matter  seemed  to  have 
been  settled.  Each  time  this  has  occurred,  someone 

'  Bremer's  encephale  isole  preparation  falls  short  of  qualifying 
as  equivalent  to  the  intact  preparation  to  the  extent  that  it 
interrupts  part  of  the  reticular  input;  however,  it  is  different 
from  the  deeply  anesthetized  animal  to  the  extent  that  part  of 
the  reticular  system  is  intact. 


6oo 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


assuming  this  to  be  true  and  predicating  a  new  study- 
on  lliis  assumption  has,  by  his  findings,  introduced 
some  new  confusion  which  has  then  required  its 
own  gradual  resolution.  We  have  now  arrived  at  an 
interesting  dilemma  in  which  we  recognize  several 
types  of  auditory  thalamocortical  projection  to  a 
wide  lateroventral  extension  of  the  auditory  area 
as  first  defined  and,  in  addition,  projection  from 
hitherto  nonauditory  thalamic  nuclei  to  a  cortical 
area  originally  considered  to  belong  to  the  somes- 
thetic  svstem  but  now  known  to  be  excitable  also  by 
sound  (and,  indeed,  Ijy  the  nonacoustic  labyrinth 
as  well).  Moreover,  these  latter-day  auditory  areas 
seem  to  play  essential  roles  in  the  mediation  of 
auditory-guided  learned  behavior.  When  we  add  to 
this  the  findings  of  Lilly  which  tend  to  blur  func- 
tional if  not  anatomical  boundaries,  we  begin  to  be 
less  impressed  than  we  once  were  with  the  possi- 
bility of  singling  out  certain  areas  whose  sole  re- 
sponsibility and  exclusive  prerogative  lie  in  the 
realm  of  auditory  integration.  On  the  other  hand, 
we  must  be  equally  careful  to  avoid  the  other  horn 
of  the  dilemma  by  keeping  in  mind  that,  however 
dim  the  boundaries  may  become,  the  areas  we  call 
auditory  do  respond  differently  to  sound  than  do 
other  cortical  areas  and  they  do  show  differences 
among  themselves. 

Auditory  Cortex  in  Primates 

The  foregoing  section  was  ba.sed  almost  entirely 
on  the  brain  of  the  cat.  Comparable  studies  on  the 
primate  brain  are  far  fewer  in  number  and  com- 
paratively lacking  in  the  area  of  behavioral  studies. 
Otherwise,  a  history  of  developing  knowledge  of  the 
primate  cortical  auditory  areas  would  parallel  that 
in  the  cat  since  neurophysiology  traditionally  uses 
the  cat  for  pilot  experiments  which,  after  trial, 
modification  and  revision,  can  be  applied  to  the 
monkey.  The  history  of  monkey  experiments  reflects 
the  greater  efficiency  which  is  made  mandatory  by 
the  expense  of  buying  monkeys  out  of  the  charac- 
teristically meager  operating  budget  for  neurophys- 
iological  studies. 

The  early  development  of  knowledge  of  the 
monkey  auditory  cortex  is  similar  to  that  of  the 
cat,  often  appearing  in  the  same  accounts,  such  as 
those  of  Ferrier,  Munk  and  Campbell.  It  will  suffice 
here  to  say  that  by  the  beginning  of  the  twentieth 
century,  inference,  extrapolation  and  inspired  guess- 
work, based  on  some  knowledge  of  human  and 
carnivore  brains,  had  implanted  firmly  and  widely 


the  belief  that  the  primate  auditory  area  is  located 
somewhere  in  the  superior  temporal  convolution. 
Fortunately  in  view  of  this,  the  facts,  as  they  subse- 
quently accumulated,  support  this  belief. 

Aside  from  the  cytoarchitectural  studies  of  the 
earlier  neurologists,  the  modern  investigation  of  the 
primate  auditory  area  may  be  said  to  begin  with  the 
studies  of  Poliak  in  1932  (72)  based  on  Marchi 
studies  of  monkey  brains  after  lesions  in  the  medial 
geniculate  body.  He  described  the  course  and 
terminations  of  the  auditory  radiations,  defining  as 
the  cortical  projection  area  thus  delineated  the 
greater  part  of  the  superior  surface  of  the  superior 
temporal  gyrus.  The  concentration  of  terminations 
was  greater  posteriorly  than  anteriorly,  the  focal 
zone  coinciding  with  an  elevation  toward  the  pos- 
terior end  of  the  concealed  face  of  the  gyrus  which 
Poliak  likened  to  Heschl's  convolution  in  man. 
Poliak  described  a  lesser  concentration  of  fibers 
which  reaches  the  lateral  face  of  the  superior  tem- 
poral gyrus. 

Walker  (105)  and  Clark  (23),  both  using  the 
method  of  retrograde  degeneration  in  the  medial 
geniculate  body  following  lesions  in  the  superior 
temporal  cortex,  are  in  general  agreement  with 
Poliak  on  the  location  of  the  projection  area  of  the 
medial  geniculate;  however,  both  outline  a  smaller 
area  confined  to  the  posterior  part  of  the  superior 
face  of  the  gyrus.  If  the  situation  in  the  monkey  is 
similar  to  that  found  in  the  cat  by  Rose  &  Woolsey 
(85),  in  which  only  A  I  of  all  the  auditory  region 
receives  essential  projection,  it  would  be  expected 
that  only  the  corresponding  area  in  the  monkey 
would  be  revealed  by  the  retrograde  degeneration 
method.  On  the  other  hand,  the  Marchi  method  in 
conjunction  with  medial  geniculate  lesions  should  in 
addition  demonstrate  some  of  the  fibers  constituting 
sustaining  projections  to  a  wider  area.  No  study  of 
the  primate  auditory  thalamocortical  relationships 
comparable  to  the  Rose  and  \V'oolsey  study  of  the 
cat  is  available.  There  are,  however,  some  hints  de- 
rived from  several  other  studies  that  similar  prin- 
ciples may  apply. 

Electrophysiological  efforts  to  map  the  primate 
auditory  cortex,  like  the  comparable  studies  of  the 
cat,  show  the  same  sort  of  progression.  They  begin 
with  a  limited  area  and,  with  improvement  of  in- 
struments and  methodology,  expand  and  become 
subdivided.  There  is,  in  the  monkey,  an  additional 
handicap  which  limited  the  accuracy  of  the  early 
studies.  This  arises  from  the  fact  the  primate  audi- 
torv  area,   unlike  the  feline,  lies  almost  entirely  in 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


60  I 


cortex  concealed  within  the  Sylvian  sulcus,  part  of 
it,  in  fact,  facing  inward  toward  the  insula.  This 
makes  necessary  some  special  preparation  in  order 
to  gain  access  for  the  exploring  electrode.  Earlier 
workers  usually  accomplished  this  by  extensive  re- 
movals of  the  overhanging  frontal  and  parietal 
operculum,  the  latter  of  which,  as  later  events  have 
shown,  actually  contains  some  auditory  responsive 
cortex.  This  was  therefore  missed  until  more  re- 
cently Pribram  and  his  coworkers  (73)  were  able  to 
expose  the  areas  in  question  without  major  destruc- 
tion of  tissue. 

The  first  electrophysiological  demonstration  of  the 
simian  auditory  cortex,  bv  Ades  &  Felder  (6),  used 
click  stimulation  and  the  usual  exploration  for 
cortical  response.  An  area  on  the  posterior  part  of 
the  superior  temporal  plane  was  found  to  Ije  re- 
sponsive; this  is  shown  in  figure  7.  This  area  is  larger 
than  those  outlined  by  Cllark  and  by  Walker  but 
confirms  the  general  location.  It  is  somewhat  smaller 
than  the  area  shown  by  Poliak  (72)  to  receive  genic- 
ulocortical  fibers. 

Licklider  (53)  and  Licklider  &  Kryter  (54),  as- 
suming the  Ades-Felder  definition  of  the  auditory 
area  to  be  correct,  explored  it  while  stimulating  with 
short  bursts  of  pure  tone.  They  were  able  to  demon- 
strate a  degree  of  specificity  of  various  parts  of  the 
area  referable  to  frequency  of  stimulation.  Bailey 
et  al.  (i  i)  defined  similar  auditory  areas  from  monkey 
and  chimpanzee,  in  each  case  confined  to  the  supra- 


FIG.  7.  View  of  monkey  brain  with  operculum  cut  away  to 
expose  supratemporal  plane.  Horizontal  shaded  area  shows  'click 
map'  of  Ades  &  Felder  (6);  crosshaiched  area  within  'click  map' 
shows  area  determined  by  Walker  by  retrograde  degeneration 
to  be  medial  geniculate  projection  area. 


temporal  plane,  and  confirmed  the  tonotopic  dis- 
tribution suggested  by  Licklider  &  Kryter.  Walzl 
(107)  and  Woolsey  (112),  using  different  methods, 
also  demonstrated  a  specificity  of  cochleocortical 
projection  in  the  same  area  but  found,  in  addition, 
a  region  of  reversed  order  of  projection  on  the  upper 
(parietal)  bank  of  the  sylvian  fi.s.sure,  thus  extending 
the  boundaries  of  auditory  cortex.  The  aspects  of 
these  and  other  studies  which  relate  to  topical  pro- 
jection, localized  response  to  different  stimulus  fre- 
quencies or  both  will  be  considered  in  more  detail 
in  a  section  dealing  specifically  with  that  aspect  of 
auditory  projection. 

The  most  extensive  auditory  area  yet  described 
for  the  monkey  is  that  of  Pribram  et  al.  (73).  They 
mapped  the  cortical  areas  from  which  electrical  re- 
sponse could  be  evoked  by  clicks,  exposing  the  depth 
of  the  Sylvian  fissure  and  the  insula  by  gently  sepa- 
rating the  lips  of  the  fissure  and  wedging  them 
apart  in  various  ways.  They  do  not  relate  in  detail 
the  means  by  which  damage  to  the  rich  vascular 
tree  of  the  middle  cerebral  artery  was  avoided; 
however,  this  surgical  tour  de  Jorce  must  have  been 
accomplished  because  the  effects  of  severe  heinor- 
rhage  and  ischemia  in  the  region  supplied  by  this 
vascular  tree  are  not  e\'ident  in  the  results.  The 
corte.x  of  the  posterior  supratemporal  plane,  superior 
temporal  gyrus,  insula  and  inferior  parietal  lobe  all 
yielded  responses  to  clicks  (fig.  8).  On  the  basis  of 
latency  of  initial  positive  deflection  and  other  cri- 
teria, the  authors  identify  (by  inference  or  direct 
statement)  subdivisions  of  the  total  responsive  area 
with  those  of  the  cat  as  follows:  /)  the  posterior 
supratemporal  plane  with  A  I;  j?)  the  anterior  margin 
of  responsive  area  of  supratemporal  plane,  posterior 
insula  and  posterior  inferior  parietal  operculum 
with  'secondary'  area  of  Ades  &  Bremer  and,  hence, 
EP  of  Rose  &  Woolsey;  and  3)  the  parietal  opercu- 
lum with  S  II.  This  analysis  omits  most  of  the  re- 
sponsive area  of  the  posterior  insula  which  corre- 
sponds roughly  to  the  'second'  auditory  area  (or 
simian  A  II)  of  Walzl  (107)  and  Woolsey  (112).  In 
this  regard  the  data  of  Pribram  et  al.  furnish  no 
parallel  to  the  Walzl  &  Woolsey  data  because  the 
definition  of  A  I  and  A  II  in  the  lexicon  of  the  latter 
two  authors  hinges  upon  the  presence  in  each  of 
cochlear  projections  of  mutually  opposite  orienta- 
tion. 

Pribram  et  al.  include  data  on  retrograde  degenera- 
tion after  lesions  of  the  posterior  supratemporal 
plane  but  not  of  any  other  part  of  their  responsive 
area.  So  far  as  this  goes,  it  confirms  the  impression 


602 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  8.  A.  Lateral  view  of  monkey  brain,  the  sylvian  fissure  spread  to  show  area  responsive  to 
clicks  Cshaded')  according  to  Pribram  et  at.  (.Ti)-  ^-  Enlarged  view  of  shaded  part  of  ^.  SL,  short  latency 
area;  PO,  parietal  operculum. 


that  the  simian  posterior  supratemporal  plane  corre- 
sponds to  the  feline  A  I  inasmuch  as  in  each  case 
destruction  of  the  area  leads  to  severe  degeneration 
of  all  but  the  caudal  portion  of  the  principal  part  of 
the  medial  geniculate  body.  Pribram  et  al.,  however, 
contrary  to  the  findings  of  Rose  &  VVoolsey,  are 
quite  positive  in  asserting  that  such  a  lesion  and 
consequent  thalamic  degeneration  effectively  elimi- 
nates all  conduction,  not  only  to  the  simian  counter- 
part of  S  II  but  also  to  the  other  normally  responsive 
cortex  of  the  ipsilateral  hemisphere.  From  this  they 
conclude  that  the  pathway  to  S  II  follows  collaterals 
from  medial  geniculate  to  some  thalamic  nucleus 
which  projects  to  S  II.  This  impression  gains  credi- 
bility from  their  oJKervation  that  the  negating  effect 
of  the  lesion  is  not  seen  acutely  (i.e.  before  conse- 
quent geniculate  degeneration  has  taken  place) 
but  only  some  weeks  later  (i.e.  after  the  effects  have 
been  felt  in  the  medial  geniculate).  This  conflict, 
except  in  the  unlikely  event  it  represents  a  species 
difference,  can  be  resolved  only  by  full  analysis  of 
the  retrograde  consequences  of  all  combinations  of 
suspected  subareas  in  each  species  combined  with 
electrophysiological  delineation  of  responsive  cortex 
in  each  instance  after  time  lapse  to  allow  degenera- 
tion and  just  before  sacrifice.  This  is  a  large  order, 
but  it  is  both  legitimate  and  feasible. 

The  reader  will  note  that  in  the  discussion  of  the 
primate  auditory  cortex,  no  functional  counterparts 
of    the    feline    insular    and    temporal    regions    have 


emerged.  This  probably  represents  merely  a  relative 
shortage  of  information  on  the  monkey,  a  lack  which 
may  be  filled  in  part  by  the  additional  information 
we  hope  will  accrue  from  the  arduous  future  study 
suggested  in  the  last  paragraph  and  in  part  from  be- 
havioral studies  comparable  to  those  in  the  cat.  The 
behavioral  studies  on  the  monkey  so  far  available 
are  of  little  use  inasmuch  as  they  show  no  positive 
loss  of  auditory  capacity  and  the  lesions  are  incom- 
plete (26,  27).  At  the  least,  in  the  outlook  on  the 
insular  and  temporal  regions  of  the  monkey,  there  is 
reason  to  hope  becau.se,  if  we  accept  the  total  area  of 
Pribram  et  al.,  a  portion  of  the  insula  and  all  of  the 
lateral  surface  of  the  posterior  superior  temporal 
gyrus  are  responsive  to  clicks  but  have  not  yet  been 
claimed  for  any  other  anatomical,  electrical  or  func- 
tional counterpart  in  the  cat. 

Finally,  we  may  note  that  the  definition  of  A  II 
in  the  monkey  rests  in  part  on  published  data  (107, 
116)  but  in  even  larger  part  on  logical  though  less 
well  supported  extension  of  that  data.  This  is  due, 
at  least  in  part,  to  relative  inaccessibility.  In  this 
connection,  it  may  have  been  noted  that  of  all  the 
subdivisions  in  the  cat's  auditory  cortex,  A  II  seems 
to  be  the  least  firmly  established.  At  no  time  since 
the  initial  definition  of  A  II  has  it  been  as  clearly 
valid  an  area  as  it  was  at  that  time.  The  experiments 
of  Kiang  (49),  which  explore  the  whole  region  in 
the  cat  by  a  combination  of  techniques,  make  the 
distinction   between   A   I   and   A   II   more  nebulous 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


603 


than  it  previously  had  seemed.  It  is  possible  this 
region  may  prove  to  be,  as  Kiang  suggests,  a  transi- 
tional area  or  a  fringe  portion  of  A  I. 


TOPOLOGIC    AND    TONOTOPIC    PROJECTION 

An  investigator  entering  upon  the  serious  stud\ 
of  the  neural  aspects  of  audition  25  years  ago  in- 
evitably found  that  the  single  most  engrossing  topic 
of  study  and  speculation  was  that  of  the  neurological 
basis  of  pitch  perception.  This  was  not  at  that  time 
a  new  tendency.,  for  von  Helmholtz  was  in  large 
part  responsible  for  initiating  it  many  years  before 
by  expressing  the  idea  that  the  basilar  membrane 
of  the  cochlea  resonated  in  different,  narrowly  re- 
stricted regions  to  different  frequenceis  of  .sound.  It 
followed  that  if  the  cochlea  is  thus  an  analyzer  of 
frequency,  it  must  be  reflected  faithfully  in  the  brain 
in  order  to  make  the  results  of  its  analytical  efforts 
available  to  conscious  processes.  There  was  little 
opportunity  to  test  this  hypothesis  rigorously  until 
the  advent,  during  the  1930's,  of  instruments  which 
would  reliably  measure  the  neural  results  of  stimula- 
tion by  sounds.  When  this  occurred,  there  was  a 
rapid  increment  of  interest  in  auditory  neurophys- 
iology and,  in  natural  consequence,  in  study  of  the 
anatomy  of  the  auditory  pathway  and  of  behavior 
as  related  to  audition.  It  was  quickly  established  that 
different  parts  of  the  cochlea  do  indeed  respond  dif- 
ferently to  different  stimulus  frequencies,  though  not 
for  the  reasons  nor  in  the  manner  which  von  Helm- 
holtz thought,  and  this  served  to  whet  interest  in  the 
central  reflection  of  the  phenomenon. 

Another  factor  in  the  rapid  acceleration  of  interest 
in  auditory  neuroanatomy  and  neurophysiology  was 
the  then  recent  demonstration  of  a  very  preci.se  point- 
to-point  projection  of  the  retina  through  the  optic 
tract  and  lateral  geniculate  body  to  the  occipital 
cortex.  It  was  conceived  that  this  sort  of  anatomical 
arrangement  might  be  characteristic  of  sensory  projec- 
tion systems  in  general.  Certainly  a  similar  orderliness 
and  topologic  precision  could  be  di.scerned  in  the 
somesthetic  system,  in  which  the  functional  counter- 
part of  the  visual  field  map  was  the  body  surface 
map.  Why  not  a  projection  of  the  organ  of  Corti 
and  a   tonal  map  for  the  acoustic  pathway? 

The  central  auditory  pathway,  unfortunately  for 
those  theories,  is  more  complex  in  its  multinucleate 
interconnections  than  the  visual  or  somesthetic  path- 
ways and  it  has  not  yielded  easily  to  being  fitted 
into  the  same  general  scheme.  Nevertheless,  the  nor- 


mal anatomy  of  the  system  is  not  without  some  indi- 
cations of  orderliness  and  a  variety  of  experimental 
techniques  has  revealed  even  more.  Likewise,  on  the 
functional  side,  no  easy  uncomplicated  scheme  of 
matching  frequencies  and  fibers  has  presented  itself. 
It  has  gradually  become  evident  that  this  system  is 
unicjue  among  sen.sory  systems  and  presents  problems 
peculiar  to  itself;  however,  it  has  also  become  more 
apparent  that  some  relationship  exists  between  ana- 
tomical location  and  location  in  the  audible  spectrum. 
Although  it  is  somewhat  awkward  to  do  so,  it  will 
be  best  to  consider  structural  and  functional  localiza- 
tion together,  and  the  story  will  ije  more  coherent 
if  we  largely  ignore  chronological   sequence. 

One  of  the  more  conspicuous  features  of  the  nerve 
supply  of  the  organ  of  Corti  and  the  termination  of 
the  cochlear  nerve  fibers  in  the  cochlear  nuclei  is 
their  orderly  anatomical  dispositions.  At  their  en- 
trance into  the  cochlear  nuclei,  the  cochlear  nerve 
fibers  bifurcate  along  a  curving  line  such  that  the 
linear  relationship  of  their  origins  in  the  cochlea  from 
apex  to  base  is  preserved  (59,  75)  with  those  from  the 
apex  bifurcating  lateroventrally,  those  from  the  base, 
dorsomedially.  The  two  branches  of  each  fiber  then 
pass  respectively  to  dorsal  and  ventral  cochlear 
nuclei  and  multiple  terminations  among  the  cells 
of  these  nuclei.  Single  unit  responses  in  the  dorsal 
cochlear  nucleus  to  pure  tone  stimuli  were  shown  by 
Galambos  &  Davis  (34,  35)  to  respond  selectively 
to  tones  of  different  frequency,  each  having  its  charac- 
teristic frequency.  Very  recently.  Rose  et  al.  (84), 
applying  a  similar  microelectrode  technique,  ex- 
plored the  dorsal  nucleus  more  systematically  and 
demonstrated  an  orderly  pattern  of  frequency  re- 
sponse, the  basic  feature  of  which  is  that  characteris- 
tic frequencies  of  the  single  units  vary  systematically 
from  high  at  the  medial  (dorsal)  edge  to  low  at  the 
lateral  (ventral)  edge.  Less  complete  data  indicate 
further  that  a  similar  arrangement  is  repeated  in 
each  of  the  two  divisions  of  the  ventral  nucleus. 
If  we  accept  for  the  moment  that  the  base  of  the 
cochlea  is  concerned  with  high  frequency  reception 
and  the  apex  with  low  (a  concept  which  will  need  to 
be  qualified  presently),  the  frequency  distribution 
in  the  cochlear  nuclei  corresponds  to  the  pattern  of 
nerve  terminations.  This  study  affords  the  first  step 
toward  an  explanation  of  the  as  yet  inexplicable  mean- 
ing of  the  elaborate  organization  of  the  cochlear 
nuclei. 

The  story  so  far  appears  to  be  simple  straightfor- 
ward testimony  in  favor  of  the  uncomplicated  hy- 
pothesis that  each  narrow  segment  of  the  organ  of 


6o4 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Corti  responds  to  a  correspondingly  narrow  frequency 
band  and  is  connected  by  its  own  group  of  nerve 
fibers  to  an  isolated  part  of  the  cochlear  nuclei. 
True,  upon  arrival  at  the  latter  stadon,  a  rather  dis- 
quieting multiplication  of  the  end  organ  seems  to 
take  place  such  that  the  organ  of  Corti  is  projected 
not  once  but  several  times  to  the  nuclei,  this  being 
accomplished  by  the  systematic  terminal  branching 
of  the  cochlear  axons.  There  are,  however,  aspects 
of  this  and  related  studies  which  are  more  fundamen- 
tally disturbing  to  the  hypothesis  in  its  .simplest 
form.  In  both  the  microelectrode  studies  cited  in  the 
preceding  paragraph,  while  the  frequency  response 
band  of  a  given  nuclear  element  was  extremely 
narrow  or  punctate  when  the  intensity  of  the  stimu- 
lus was  near  threshold,  as  the  intensity  was  increased 
the  band  became  progressively  wider,  especially 
toward  the  low  end  of  the  scale.  (There  was  little  or 
no  expansion  of  the  response  band  to  higher  fre- 
quencies,   even    at    \ery    high    intensity.) 

When  information  on  microelectrode  studies  of 
cochlear  nerve  fibers  became  available  (99,  100,  loi), 
one  of  the  more  striking  features  was  the  exaggeration 
of  the  principle  just  described  for  the  second-order 
units  in  the  cochlear  nucleus.  The  individual  units 
in  the  nerve  also  showed  sharply  restricted  frequency 
specificity  at  threshold  intensities  but,  upon  increasing 
intensity,  each  fiber  responded  to  a  wider  and  wider 
range  of  lower  frequencies  but  not  to  higher.  Instead 
of  responding  each  to  its  own  frequency,  therefore, 
it  would  be  more  correct  to  say  that  each  fiber  re- 
sponds to  all  frequencies  up  to  its  high  frequency 
limit  and  to  none  higher.  This  fits  well  with  direct 
observation  by  von  Bekesy  of  cutoff  points  of  vibra- 
tion of  the  basilar  membrane  which  vary  with  fre- 
quency of  stimulus  and  involve  all  of  the  membrane 
up  to  the  cutoff  point  (99,   too). 

If  the  feature  of  auditory  nerve  function  just  de- 
scribed is  true,  then  random  partial  lesions  of  the 
nerve  should  not,  as  was  once  supposed,  result  in 
hearing  loss  in  the  form  of  tonal  islands  but  instead 
in  losses  at  the  highest  frequencies  with  smaller 
lesions  and  a  progressive  high  frequency  loss  as  more 
and  more  fibers  are  involved.  Relatively  few  fibers 
are  stimulated  by  high  tones,  and  so  high  frequencies 
are  most  vulnerable  since  in  the  spiral  course  of  the 
nerve  bundles  also,  it  is  inconceivable  that  any 
appreciable  lesion  could  miss  these.  Hence,  in  lesions 
sparing  only  a  few  fibers,  hearing  should  be  preserved 
only  for  tones  at  the  low  end  of  the  spectrum  (since 
most  or  all  fibers  are  sensitive  to  low  frequencies, 
and   the  lower   tones  are   therefore  relatively  invul- 


nerable to  any  but  complete  section  of  the  nerve). 
This  is,  in  fact,  the  common  finding  in  both  animal 
and  human  studies  of  this  kind  (41,  66,  91,  92,  93). 

Studies  of  localized  frequency  response  of  neural 
elements  in  stations  lying  between  the  cochlear  nuclei 
and  the  auditory  cortex  are  relatively  few  in  number, 
although  they  are  increasing  currently.  They  rely 
mainly  on  the  microelectrode  techniques.  Such  studies 
have  been  made  of  the  superior  olivary  nuclear  com- 
plex (33,  99),  the  inferior  colliculus  (99,  102)  and 
the  medial  geniculate  body  (31,  40).  All  of  these 
share  with  each  other  and  with  the  studies  on  cochlear 
nerve  and  nuclei  the  finding  of  elements  which  can 
be  activated  by  tonal  stimuli;  of  others,  already 
discharging  spontaneously,  whose  rate  of  (spike) 
discharge  is  increased  by  tonal  stimuli;  and  of  still 
others,  already  discharging,  whose  activity  is  in- 
hibited by  stimulation.  An  exception  is  seen  in  the 
work  of  Tasaki  &  Davis  (100,  loi)  who  found  no 
fibers  in  the  cochlear  nerve  whose  acti\ity  was 
inhibited  by  stimulation. 

The  response  band  or  area  seems  to  undergo  some 
change  in  shape  at  successively  higher  stations.  In 
the  nerve,  it  is  characterized  by  a  sharp  high  fre- 
quency cutoff  and  a  long  extension  into  lower  fre- 
quency range.  The  cochlear  nuclear  elements  show 
high  frequency  cutoff  nearly  as  sharp  as  nerve  ele- 
ments but  with  a  lesser  expansion  of  the  area  into 
lower  frequencies  at  higher  intensities  (31,  99,   loi). 

An  abstract  report  of  single  unit  recording  from  the 
several  subdivisions  of  the  superior  olivary  complex 
has  just  appeared  (33),  and  a  brief  account  of  simi- 
lar though  less  extensive  experiments  is  included 
together  with  those  on  other  nuclei  (99).  There  is  a 
greater  variety  of  responsive  units  in  these  cell  masses 
than  in  the  cochlear  nuclei,  their  relati\e  numbers 
varving  with  location  in  the  subdivisions  and  also 
with  other  factors.  Some  units  (from  the  reports  it  is 
not  clear  what  percentage)  respond  differentially  to 
tonal  stimuli.  The  response  area  of  a  given  unit 
resembles  closely  those  of  cochlear  nuclear  units  in 
that  the  high  frequency  cutoff  is  still  sharp  and  the 
degree  of  extension  into  low  frequency  range  about 
the  same  as  for  the  nucleus.  Sumi  et  al.  (99)  report 
that  they  found  trapezoid  elements  responding  to 
tones  over  20  kc  situated  rostrally,  those  to  tones  be- 
low 300  cps  caudally,  and  between  these,  5000  and 
3000  cps  elements  side  by  side.  Thus  there  is  indica- 
tion of  tonotopic  localization  of  the  projection  thus 
far. 

The  inferior  colliculus  (99,  102)  shows  some  dif- 
ferences and   some   similarities   to   the   lower  centei 


CENTRAL    AUDITOR^-    MECHANISMS 


605 


with  respect  to  single  unit  responses.  The  response 
areas  are  much  narrower  than  in  the  more  caudally 
situated  nuclei.  The  threshold  is  just  as  sharp,  but 
one  is  less  impressed  with  the  high  frequency  cutoff, 
there  being  some  tendency  for  band  width  to  widen 
toward  the  higher  as  well  as  the  lower  tones  with  in- 
crease in  intensity,  though  the  low  tone  bias  is  still 
prominent.  The  inferior  colliculus  has  not  been  sys- 
tematically explored,  so  we  do  not  know  to  what  rela- 
tive degree  each  part  of  this  complex  organ  may  be 
populated  with  frequency-selective  units.  We  can 
only  be  sure  that  such  units  can  l)e  found  in  consider- 
able numbers. 

The  medial  geniculate  body  responsive  elements 
show,  again,  some  similarities  and  some  differences 
to  the  situation  in  the  medullary  nuclei.  While  there 
are  many  units  which  can  be  stimulated  by  pure 
tones,  there  are  also  many  which  cannot,  though  the 
latter  group  includes  many  units  which  do  respond 
to  clicks,  noise  or  both.  Of  those  responding  to  pure 
tones,  it  is  noted  the  frequency  ijands  to  which  they 
respond  are  broader  at  threshold  than  those  of  the 
other  nuclei.  Furthermore,  the  bands  widen,  with 
increasing  intensity,  almost  equally  toward  higher 
and  lower  tones.  The  available  data  offer  us  little 
or  nothing  which  would  point  to  the  existence  of  a 
nuclear  plan  or  map  of  frequency-specific  areas; 
however,  this  is  really  an  open  question  which  can 
be  .settled  only  by  a  more  systematic  survey  of  fre- 
quency-biased (if  not  specific)  units  throughout  the 
nucleus. 

A  recent  study  of  auditory  cortical  single  unit 
response  to  pure  tone  (25)  has  demonstrated  some 
units  (few  relative  to  brain-stem  nuclei)  which  are 
responsive  to  tonal  stimuli  (as  well  as  others  respon- 
sive to  other  auditory  stimuli).  Tone-sensitive  units 
are  said  to  be  usually  maximally  sensitive  within  a 
restricted  frequency  band,  this  band  widening  rela- 
tively little  as  intensity  increases.  In  the  terms  we 
have  been  using,  the  response  area  follows  the  pro- 
gressive tendency  for  narrowing  of  the  band  width 
overall  while  widening  it  somewhat  at  threshold. 
It  should  be  emphasized  that  among  the  units  sensi- 
tive to  any  kind  of  auditory  stimulus,  which  alto- 
gether constitute  less  than  60  per  cent  of  all  units 
identified,  those  sensitive  to  pure  tone  represent 
only  a  small  fraction.  With  respect  to  location  of 
frequency-specific  units,  the  findings  indicate  that 
those  most  sensitive  to  low  frequencies  predominate 
in  the  posterior  A  I  field  and  high  frequency  units 
predominate  in  anterior  A  I,  although  in  neither  case 


is    the    characteristic    t\pe    the    exclusive    frequency 
sensitive  type. 

In  review  of  the  studies  cited  so  far,  it  can  be  said 
that  the  sharply  restricted  frequency  specificity  of 
fibers  for  threshold  intensity  in  the  cochlear  nerve 
persists  in  units  of  the  medullary  auditory  nuclei  but 
in  the  thalamus  and  cortex  gives  way  to  restricted 
though  broader  bands  of  threshold  sensitivity.  On 
the  other  hand,  the  very  broad  frequency  response 
for  tones  below  the  high  frequency  cutoff,  characteris- 
tic of  the  cochlear  nerve  fibers,  diminishes  steadily 
in  width  as  we  ascend  the  pathway  and  the  sharp 
high  frequency  cutoff  is  lost.  Thus,  a  given  auditory 
nerve  fiber  may  respond  at  threshold  only  to  2000 
cps  tone  (for  example)  and,  with  increasing  intensity 
may  respond  to  all  tones  lower  than  2000  cps,  but  will 
respond  to  no  higher  tones,  no  matter  how  intense; 
however,  a  cortical  unit  may  respond  at  threshold 
to  a  restricted  band  centering  at  2000  cps  but  re- 
spond at  considerably  higher  intensity  to  a  band  not 
much  wider.  From  the  situation  in  the  cochlear  nerve 
where  virtually  all  fibers  are  sensitive  to  tonal  stim- 
uli, we  go  to  that  in  the  cortex  where  only  a  fraction 
of  the  total  elements  are  tone  sensitive.  Although  we 
do  not  have  very  exact  information  on  the  percentage 
of  tone-sensitive  elements  at  all  levels,  the  indications 
are  that  there  is  a  proportional  decrea.se,  but  no  ac- 
tual numerical  decrease,  and,  more  probably,  some 
increase  of  elements  whose  prime  preoccupation  has 
to  do  with   stimulus   frequency. 

Postponing  for  the  moment  any  interpretation  of 
the  microelectrode  studies,  let  us  turn  to  other  studies 
in  which  the  basic  questions  have  to  do  with  overall 
specificity  of  projection  rather  than  that  of  indi\idual 
fibers  or  cells  of  the  pathway.  Both  types  of  evidence 
will  have  to  be  incorporated  into  any  effort  at  inter- 
pretation. 

We  have  followed  the  upward  progress  of  single 
tone-sensitive  elements  of  the  projection  pathway 
and  noted  that  through  the  several  synapses  and  proc- 
essing centers,  a  certain  change  in  character  of  rela- 
tive frequency  sensitivity  has  occurred  together  with 
a  dispersion  of  these  elements  among  others  which 
seem  to  have  different  concerns.  It  would  now  be 
well  to  examine  the  overall  situation  to  determine  if 
there  is,  indeed,  any  pattern  of  anatomical  or  ph\sio- 
logical  integration  which  can  be  discerned  by  correla- 
tion of  elements  or  groups  of  elements  at  one  end  of 
the  system  with  those  of  the  other.  Several  relevant 
studies  on  cat,  dog  and  monkey  at  once  present  them- 
selves. 

Let  us  consider  first  those  primarily  concerned  with 


6o6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


anatomical  integrity  of  projection.  We  have  in  the 
report  of  Woolsey  &  Walzl  (11:5)  a  pure  example 
of  anatomical  study  of  the  neural  projection  of  the 
cells  of  the  spiral  ganglion  to  the  cortical  projection 
area,  by  physiological  means  uncontaminated  by  the 
mechanical  characteristics  of  the  end  organ.  The  only 
element  of  control  lacking  will  he  apparent  as  the 
experiment  is  briefly  described.  The  procedure  was: 
/)  to  expose  the  cochlear  duct  by  removal  of  the  ex- 
ternal bony  cap.sule,  in  the  process  of  which  the  organ 
of  Corti  and  basilar  membrane  were  removed  to 
expose  the  ends  of  the  peripheral  processes  of  the 
ganglion  cell  in  the  free  border  of  the  osseous  spiral 
lamina;  and  2)  to  stimulate  by  brief  electric  shocks 
small  groups  of  these  fibers  while  exploring  the  sus- 
pected cortical  areas  for  the  responses  and  map  these. 
The  only  methodological  fault  (which  was  unavoid- 
able) is  that,  while  the  stimulus  is  electrically  and 
geographically  speaking  quite  localized,  not  all  of 
the  nerve  fibers  at  any  such  local  spot  are.  Some  of 
them  innervate  only  the  inner  hair  cells  immediately 
beyond  the  position  of  the  stimulating  electrode, 
but  the  rest  innervate  more  widespread  groups  of 
outer  hair  cells,  and  the  latter,  to  an  unknown  de- 
gree, mav  extend  the  cortical  response  area  of  a  given 
point  or  perhaps  blur  its  edges. 

On  the  basis  of  these  experiments,  Woolsey  & 
Walzl  postulated  a  double  cortical  projection  area 
(A  I  and  A  II  as  they  have  since  come  to  be  called 
and  have  been  referred  to  in  this  chapter).  Within 
A  I,  they  found  the  basal  end  of  the  cochlea  to  project 
to  the  anterior  part  of  A  I  and  the  apical  to  posterior 
A  I,  with  intermediate  cochlear  stations  projecting 
in  orderly  fashion  between.  A  II  showed  a  similar 
pattern  except  it  was  inverted,  so  that  the  basal 
cochlea  is  represented  in  posterior  A  II,  the  apical 
in  anterior  A  II;  however,  it  was  less  easy  to  trace 
the  intermediate  loci  between  these  two  focal  regions. 
Part  of  this  difficulty  could  be  due  to  the  fact  the  evi- 
dence is  limited  to  the  half  of  each  cochlear  spiral 
which  can  be  surgically  exposed,  while  the  situation 
on  the  still  inaccessible  obverse  turns  must  be  logi- 
cally inferred  without  demonstration 

We  know  from  the  foregoing  that  there  is  a  direct 
relation  of  cochlear  region  to  cortical  area.  Inferen- 
tially,  we  can  postulate  further  from  this  work  that 
high  tones  (basal  cochlea)  should  e.xcite  anterior  A  I 
and  posterior  A  II,  while  low  tones  should  be  repre- 
sented in  posterior  A  I  and  anterior  A  II.  This  has 
been  experimentally  tested  by  several  investigators. 
The  recent  work  of  Erulkar  et  al.  (25),  using  the  micro- 
electrode    method,     has    alreadv    i)een    mentioned. 


With  macroelectrode  techniques,  the  cortical  re- 
sponse to  sustained  pure  tones  (or  noise  for  that  mat- 
ter) is  less  conspicuous  than  one  might  have  thought, 
and  it  is  difficult  to  evaluate  reliably  for  purposes  of 
mapping  areas  excitable  by  sound.  This  applies  to 
the  sound  after  it  has  been  turned  on  during  what 
Rosenblith  calls  a  quasi-stationary  state,  the  prefix, 
'quasi',  in  this  case  representing  the  overwhelming 
burden  of  our  ignorance  of  the  continuously  changing 
'  backgroimd'  electrical  activity,  and  the  ways  it  may 
be  influenced  to  change  further  by  sound.  The  same 
handicap  does  not  apply  to  the  onset  response  to 
an\-  kind  of  sound  stimulus  (onset  can  be  seen  as  a 
high-voltage  wave  response,  due  presumably  to 
arrival  at  cortex  of  a  surge  excitation),  a  fact  which 
has  been  capitalized  in  the  use  of  clicks,  which  are 
brief  complex  noises,  and  of  tonal  pips,  which  are 
brief  pure  tones  in  which  the  frequency  characteris- 
tics are  established  and  brought  to  threshold  intensity 
within  a  very  few  cycles.  Response  to  tonal  pips  has 
been  used  to  map  frequency-sensitiv'e  cortical  areas 
and  so  has  another  method,  the  evoked  strychnine 
spike  technique.  The  latter  depends  upon  the  fact 
that  a  small  part  of  the  auditory  cortical  area  can  be 
.sensitized  with  strychnine  so  that  onset  response  of  a 
tone  to  which  the  area  is  normally  sensitive  evokes  a 
strychnine  spike  which,  unlike  the  response  of  the 
untreated  cortex,  is  so  characteristic  it  cannot  lie 
lost  in  the  background  activity. 

In  earlier  efforts  at  mapping  the  auditory  cortex 
with  respect  to  differential  frequency  sensitivity, 
both  in  cat  and  monkey,  the  tonal  pip  and  tonal  on- 
set methods  were  used,  recording  from  the  un- 
treated auditory  cortex  in  anesthetized  animals. 
In  the  cat  (53),  Licklider  found  that  rough  focal  areas 
of  maximal  response  to  higher  or  lower  frequencies 
could  be  found  which  are  in  general  agreement  with 
the  more  recent  stud\'  of  Hind  (44)-  Licklider  felt 
the  situation  could  be  better  described  in  terms  of 
gradients  rather  than  restricted  tonal  foci  because 
of  the  extensive  overlapping. 

Hind  (44),  using  the  evoked  strychnine  method, 
presented  more  extensive  data  and  extended  the  fre- 
quency range  studied.  (It  should  be  noted  that  the 
technique  was  originalh'  worked  out  by  Tunturi 
(103)  and  applied  to  study  of  the  dog's  auditory 
cortex.  Tunturi's  work  will  not  be  described  here 
because,  while  in  general  agreement  with  others, 
the  comparison  of  data  on  dog  and  cat  is  troublesome 
due  to  configural  differences  in  the  brain.  We  will, 
therefore,  to  conserve  space  and  avoid  confusion, 
confine  the  discussion  to  the  cat  studies  which  are 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


607 


more  useful  by  virtue  of  wide  comparability  with 
others.)  Hind  found  areas  showing  predilection  for 
higher  and  lower  frequencies.  There  is  general 
agreement  with  Licklidcr's  frequency  map  but, 
whereas  the  latter's  highest  tested  frequency  was  8 
kc.  Hind's  study  goes  as  high  as  50  kc.  Furthermore, 
Hind  found  two  high  frequency  areas,  namely  an- 
terior A  I  and  posterior  AH,  and  two  low  frequency 
areas,  namely  posterior  A  I  and  anterior  A  H.  On 
both  A  I  and  A  H,  the  area  between  high  and  low- 
could  be  spoken  of  only  as  middle  frequency  range 
area,  the  data  permitting  no  finer  gradation.  Hind's 
findings  seem  to  agree  with  those  of  Woolsey  & 
Walzl  (113).  Together,  they  indicate  a  broad  cor- 
respondence between  cochlea  and  cortical  projection 
on  the  one  hand  and  stimulus  frequency  and  cortical 
frequency  .sensitivity  on  the  other.  Let  us  note  that 
the  data  do  not  permit  us  to  think  here  of  a  finely 
tuned  system. 

It  is  interesting  to  note  that  Hind  was  able,  at  each 
cortical  point  studied,  by  varying  both  frequency 
and  intensity  of  stimulus,  to  outline  areas  of  response 
which  look  very  much  like  those  of  single  units  in  the 
microelectrode  studies.  The  focus  of  threshold  fre- 
quency is  not  as  sharp,  and  the  response  area  widens 
rapidly  both  up  and  down  the  scale. 

Similar  studies  to  those  in  the  cat  are  available  for 
the  monkey  and  the  results,  which  will  not  be  pre- 
sented in  detail,  are  similar.  Woolsey  (112)  and  Walzl 
(107)  repeated  on  monkeys  their  earlier  experiments 
on  cats  with  similar  results  (cf.  preceding  section). 
Licklider  &  Kryter  (54)  described  a  pattern  of  fre- 
quency representation  showing  low  frequencies  to- 
ward the  anterior  part  of  the  auditory  area  of  the 
supratemporal  plane  (refer  to  fig.  8  for  orientation) 
grading  to  high  frequencies  most  posteriorly.  This 
general  arrangement  was  confirmed  by  Bailey  et  al. 
(11).  Kennedy  (47)  explored  the  monkey's  temporal 
region  according  to  the  same  general  plan  as  in  Hind's 
study  on  the  cat.  She  found  no  widespread  response 
to  tonal  stimuli  comparaijle  to  that  responsive  to 
clicks  described  by  Pribram  et  al.  (73),  although  she 
confirmed  their  findings  with  click  stimulation. 
Kennedy  did  find  the  presumptive  monkey  A  I 
area  of  the  supratemporal  plane  respon.sive  to  tonal 
onset  which,  enhanced  by  strychnine,  yielded  fre- 
quency intensity  thresholds  for  each  point  similar 
to  those  of  Hind.  Her  composite  map  of  frequency 
representation  generally  confirms  but  also  extends 
(with  respect  to  both  area  and  frequency  range) 
the  study  of  Licklider  &  Kryter.  It  also  considerably 
sharpens  the  picture  for  the  monkey  and  shows  the 


pattern  to  conform  to  a  plan  of  concentric  octave 
bands,  each  oriented  from  medial  to  lateral.  As  in 
the  cat,  the  overlapping  of  frequency  range  areas  is 
at  least  as  impressive  as  their  .separation,  but  the  gen- 
eral trend  is  perhaps  more  cleAr-cut  than  in  the  cat; 
howe\'er,  Kennedy  shows  no  A  II  and  this  helps  to 
to  make  the  results  look  cleaner  as  compared  to 
Hind's. 

Summary  and  Discussion  of  Tupalogic 
and  Tonotopic  Projection 

It  can  be  taken  as  settled  that  a  degree  of  frequency 
specificity  is  characteristic  of  some  of  the  neurons  of 
the  central  acoustic  system.  In  numbers,  the.se  vary 
from  a  great  many  elements  in  the  cochlear  nerve, 
through  progressively  diininishing  percentages  of 
the  total  at  intermediate  recording  stations,  to  an 
undetermined  but  certainly  small  proportion  of 
elements  in  the  auditory  cortex.  We  must  also  reduce 
the  term  'specificity'  to  its  real  proportions,  a  quali- 
fication which  has  often  not  been  made  in  interpre- 
ting this  kind  of  data.  The  term  really  applies  well 
only  if  we  are  talking  about  threshold  intensity  and, 
even  with  this  qualification,  it  applies  best  to  the 
more  caudally  situated  recording  stations  rather  than 
to  the  thalamus  or  cortex.  A  second  aspect  of  this 
specificity  has  to  do  with  the  manner  and  degree  of 
expansion  of  the  respon.se  area  with  increasing  in- 
tensity. At  the  nerve,  and  to  an  only  slightly  lesser 
degree  in  the  cochlear  nuclei,  the  direction  of  the  ex- 
pansion is  strictly  toward  the  lower  part  of  the  scale 
and  in  degree  is  so  wide  as  to  make  us  think  that  some 
fibers  are  stimulated  by  high  tones,  many  by  inter- 
mediate tones  and  virtually  all  by  low  tones,  given  a 
stimulus  of  sufficient  intensity.  As  we  ascend  this 
changes,  so  that  among  those  cortical  elements  which 
are  sensitive  to  tone  each,  though  less  sharply  tuned 
at  threshold,  is  comparatively  greatly  restricted  in 
range  of  frequency  sensitivity  even  at  quite  high 
intensity  and  expansion  of  response  area  is  both 
up  and  down  the  scale.  Thus,  while  'attention'  to  the 
parameter  of  frequency  is  evident  froin  cochlea  to 
cortex,  the  original  coding  of  this  information,  im- 
posed by  the  mechanical  characteristics  of  the  coch- 
lea, is  changed,  perhaps  in  the  cochlear  nuclei,  per- 
haps aided  by  the  superior  olivary  complex,  perhaps 
even  more  gradually,  so  that  in  the  more  rostral 
parts  of  the  pathway,  tone-sensitive  elements  are  in 
one  way  even  more  frequency-selective  than  those  in 
the  nerve. 

We  have  spoken  of  progressive  dispersion  of  tone- 


6o8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


sensitive  elements  in  tiie  upper  reaches  of  the  audi- 
tory pathway.  This  may  be  a  misleading  term.  What 
we  really  mean  is  that  such  elements  become  less  and 
less  predominant  from  the  standpoint  of  numbers, 
or  to  state  it  another  way,  there  is  a  progressively 
increasing  proportion  of  elements  which  have  no 
direct  concern  with  the  parameter  of  stimulus  fre- 
quency. Many  of  these,  however,  do  respond  (as  do 
all  the  tone-sensitive  units)  to  complex  sounds  (e.g. 
clicks,  noise)  encompassing  wide  bands  of  the  audible 
spectrum.  We  do  not  know  why  this  is  true  but,  within 
the  bounds  of  known  facts,  it  is  not  difficult  to  en- 
vision a  theoretical  switching  system  by  which  tone 
and  noise  sensitive  and  only  noi.se-sensitive  elements 
would  come  to  exist  in  parallel.  We  know  that  the 
cochlear  nuclei  contain  at  least  three  projections  or 
replicas  of  the  organ  of  Corti.  According  to  the  micro- 
electrode  evidence  (84),  these  retain  the  original 
coding  of  the  cochlea;  however,  in  the  output  of  the 
nuclear  neurons,  there  is  no  inherent  constraint  to 
preserve  the  original  coding  pattern.  If,  in  the  ensu- 
ing relays,  we  picture  one  group  of  cells  each  of  which 
receives  approximately  equal  synaptic  terminations 
from  all  parts  of  one  cochlear  replica,  it  would  pre- 
sumably be  responsive  to  summated  volleys  from  a 
sufhciently  wide  band  of  the  replica,  regardless  of 
position  on  the  replica,  and  would  therefore  be  re- 
sponsive to  stimuli  of  frequency  bands  of  a  given 
minimal  width,  regardless  of  position  on  spectrum, 
but  not  to  narrow  bands  or  pure  tones.  A  second  group 
of  cells,  each  receiving  sufficiently  concentrated  synap- 
tic terminations  from  a  restricted  part  of  the  cochlear 
replica,  would  respond  to  stimuli  of  frequency  bands 
of  narrow  proportions  or  e\en  to  pure  tones.  If  in 
succeeding  relays  the  units  of  the  second  group  began 
to  overlap  each  other  somewhat,  we  would  expect  to 
find  the  kind  of  changes  which  in  fact  ha\e  been  found 
in  the  response  area  of  successively  higher  single 
auditory  units,  namely  some  widening  at  threshold 
but  without  a  corresponding  widening  at  higher  in- 
tensity where   small   differences   are   insignificant. 

That  the  units  showing  sensitivit\'  to  the  various 
segments  of  the  audible  spectrum  retain,  in  the  main, 
their  positions  relative  to  each  other  in  the  ascent  to 
the  cortex  is  evident,  although  there  is  also  reason 
to  believe  there  is  some  degree  of  dispersion.  This  is 
in  accord  with  the  observation,  direct  or  incidental 
by  a  variety  of  methods  of  many  in\estigators  on 
several  segments  of  the  auditory  pathway  and  on  the 
whole  pathway  from  cochlea  to  cortex,  that  the  ar- 
rangement of  auditory  elements,  though  often  intri- 
cate with  respect  to  nuclear  organization,  is  always 


orderly  and  maintains  spatial  relationships  quite 
faithfully.  Macroelectrode  studies  of  relative  sensi- 
tivity of  diflTerent  regions  of  the  auditory  cortex  con- 
firm the  impression  that,  relatively  at  least,  tone- 
sensitive  elements  of  similar  frequency  range  tend 
roughl)  to  group,  though  not  to  segregate  themselves, 
and  maintain  an  orderlv  relationship  to  elements  of 
different  frequency  characteristics.  These  studies 
also  accord  well  with  those  which  demonstrate  projec- 
tion of  the  cochlea  to  the  cortex  in  a  recognizable 
pattern. 

The  less  careful  reader  might,  at  this  ]3oint,  feel 
we  have  established  a  good  case  for  the  primary  rela- 
tionship of  frequency  specificit}'  and  anatomical  order 
and  for  these  conjointly  as  the  prime  organizational 
feature  of  the  auditory  system.  It  must  be  re-empha- 
sized that  frequency  tuning  of  auditory  neural  ele- 
ments and  of  the  o\erall  grouping  of  these  as  meas- 
ured by  electrical  response  is  relatively  fine  only  at 
threshold  intensity  and  at  higher  intensity  is  an  even 
less  con\incing  feature  when  compared  to  the  pre- 
cision of  the  psychophysical  phenomenon  of  pitch 
discrimination  to  which,  presumably,  we  must  relate 
it.  We  can  only  suppose  that  the  neurophysiological 
facts  so  far  known  reveal  to  us  only  a  part  of  the  pic- 
ture. 

Efforts  to  translate  the  anatomicophysiological 
phenomenon  of  tonotopic  projection  into  terms  of 
hearing  in  animal  experiments  have  been  discourag- 
ing but  possibly  needlessly  so.  There  is  good  reason 
to  believe  the  meager  success  of  such  ventures  is  the 
consequence  of  having  asked  the  wrong  questions, 
these  in  turn  growing  out  of  unwarranted  assumptions. 
One  doulile  assumption  of  this  sort  is  that  pitch  dis- 
crimination, of  necessity,  must  depend  upon  intact 
auditory  cortical  function  because  it  is  a  'complex' 
auditory  function.  Neither  part  of  this  is  necessarily 
true.  Compared  to  deficits  of  human  auditory  func- 
tion resulting  from  temporal  lobe  lesions,  pitch  dis- 
crimination would  be  on  the  simple  side.  Deficits 
in  human  subjects  are  not  conspicuous  unless  they 
involve  actual  auditory  aphasia  or  unless  the  patient 
is  subjected  to  rigorous  testing  which  goes  far  beyond 
routine  audiometry.  It  might  also  be  pointed  out 
that  it  is  impossible  to  prove  the  presence  of  a  com- 
plex auditory  deficit  such  as  auditory  aphasia  unless 
one  can  first  establish  that  basic  perception  is  essen- 
tially intact.  The  only  auditory  function  which  seems 
to  have  been  clearly  tied  to  the  cortex  in  aniinals, 
namely  discrimination  between  two  three-tone  pat- 
terns, would  appear  off  hand  to  be  of  a  very  diflTerent 
order  of  integrati\c  complexity  than  aphasia.   Sup- 


CENTRAL  AUDITORY   MECHANISMS 


609 


posing,  for  argument,  that  pitch  discrimination  is  a 
'complex  function',  it  still  does  not  follow  that  the 
auditory  cortex  is  the  only  or  even  the  best  neural 
matrix  in  which  the  discrimination  may  be  made. 
If  we  consider  the  possibility  that  a  kind  of  signal  to 
noise  ratio  operates  between  frequency-sensitive  and 
nonfrequency-sensitive  elements,  then  the  auditory 
cortex  affords  the  poorest  ratio  of  any  part  of  the  sys- 
tem. Finally,  the  animal  behavioral  experiments  may 
be  clouded  to  the  extent  that  the  learning  and  reten- 
tion and  conditioned  response  aspects  of  the  method 
are  neurologically  inseparable  from  the  purely  audi- 
tory aspects. 


OTHER   .ASPECTS   OF   CENTRAL   AUDITORY   FUNCTION 

If  it  is  apparent  that  the  auditory  system  contains 
a  tonotopic  organizational  pattern,  it  is  equally  ap- 
parent it  is  not  filled  by  this  pattern.  Like  many  paths 
through  a  jungle,  this  tonotopic  path  through  the 
auditory  system  has  been  found  only  because  it  was 
suspected  and  sought.  It  is  also  well  to  consider  the 
jungle  where  other  matters  may  be  equally  signifi- 
cant. By  far  the  greater  number  of  neural  elements 
in  the  system  cannot  be  demonstrated  to  have  any- 
thing to  do  with  this  parameter  (frequency)  of  the 
acoustic  stimulus.  Such  has  been  the  preoccupation 
with  it,  however,  that  any  discussion  of  audition  is 
inevitably  dominated  by  it.  Nevertheless,  some  in- 
vestigative attention  has,  in  fact,  been  otherwise 
directed  and  more  should  be.  The  remainder  of  the 
chapter  will  be  devoted  to  several  other  aspects  of 
central  auditory  function  which  have  received  some 
and  require  more  attention. 

Loudness 

The  neurophysiological  correlates  of  loudness 
probably  cannot  be  altogether  divorced  from  those 
of  pitch,  althoua;h  the  subject  was  avoided  almost 
entirely  in  the  preceding  section.  The  main  reason 
for  this  was  the  desire  to  a\oid  confusion  of  issues  in 
an  area  where  much  more  is  known  of  one  side  of  the 
issue  than  of  the  other.  A  second,  and  hardly  less 
compelling  reason,  howeser,  is  the  status  of  our  ideas 
about  the  neural  mediation  of  loudness  which  is 
currently  as  or  more  confused  than  at  any  time  in 
recent  years. 

Traditionally,  loudness  has  been  regarded,  rather 
vaguely,  as  being  expressed  in  terms  of  quantitv  of 
excitation.   Whereas  frequency  was  supposed   to  in- 


volve the  appropriate  restricted  group  of  fibers, 
loudness  was  supposed  to  be  expressed  in  terms  of  a 
greater  or  lesser  proportion  of  the  total  cross  .section 
of  pathway  excited.  With  the  realization  that,  at 
least  with  respect  to  the  cochlea,  the  total  amount  of 
end  organ  being  stimulated  and  total  number  as  well 
as  site  of  origin  of  nerve  fibers  are  involv^ed  in  the 
analysis  of  frequency,  it  became  apparent  that  the 
same  device  could  not  be  used  simultaneously  for  the 
factor  of  loudness,  at  least  in  a  simple  way.  The  pos- 
sibility exists,  however,  that  some  interaction  be- 
tween inner  and  outer  hair  cells  may  lie  at  the  bottom 
of  the  mechanical  aspect  of  loudness.  Whatever  may 
be  the  cochlear,  nerve  and  cochlear  nuclear  corre- 
lates of  loudness,  if  the  hypothesis  proposed  in  the 
preceding  discussion  relative  to  a  frequency  recoding 
function  of  the  nuclei  is  at  all  correct,  the  mediation 
of  loudness  might  also  take  a  different  form  in  the 
ascending  pathway. 

It  would  probably  be  a  mistake  to  suppose  that 
loudness  could  be  subserved  by  any  of  the  possible 
upward  projecting  patterns  without  the  addition  of  a 
factor  of  selective  neural  inhibition  by  recurrent  ele- 
ments. There  is  little  evidence  to  call  upon  in  this 
respect,  and  the  possible  significance  of  such  elements 
will  be  discussed  below  in  more  general  terms.  For 
the  moment,  we  can  only  exercise  caution  in  theoriz- 
ing about  the  mediation  of  loudness,  bearing  in  mind 
that  there  is  a  large  factor  of  relativity  inherent  in 
the  concept  and  hoping  that  further  investigation 
of  the  cochlear  end  organ  will  yield  some  suggestion 
as  to  direction. 

Laterality  of  Projection 

One  of  the  more  distinctive  features  of  the  central 
acoustic  system,  as  contrasted  to  other  .sen.sory  sys- 
tems, is  its  tendency  to  bilateral  reduplication  and 
its  bewildering  array  of  commi.s.sural  opportunities. 
It  is  obvious  the  system  begins  with  two  ears  which 
are  situated  on  opposite  sides  of  the  head,  the  open- 
ings of  the  external  auditory  meatuses  180°  apart 
in  terms  of  direction  of  orientation,  plus  or  minus 
what  few  degrees  of  bias  may  theoretically  be  im- 
posed by  the  presence  of  the  pinna.  This  immediately 
suggests  the  possibility  that  source  and  direction  of 
sound  may  be  perceived  in  part  as  a  consequence  of 
the  relative  time  of  arrival  or  loudness  or  both  of 
signal  for  the  two  ears.  From  psychophysical  studies, 
it  is  clear  that  directionality  is  one  of  the  properties 
of  sound  perception.  It  is  not  the  function  of  this 
chapter  to  go  extensively  into  the  analysis  of  direc- 


6io 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tionality  as  a  function  of  binaural  interaction;  how- 
ever, whatever  may  be  the  mechanism  of  such  interac- 
tion, it  is  our  concern  to  discuss  whether,  to  what 
extent  and  by  what  means  binaural  interaction  is 
reflected  in  the  anatomicophysiologic  organization 
of  the  central  acoustic  mechanisms.  The  evidence 
relevant  to  these  questions  is  limited  but  does  yield 
some  useful  information  which  we  will  examine 
presently. 

A  second  question  has  to  do  with  the  extent  to 
which  each  ear  is  bilaterally  represented  in  the  pro- 
jection pathway.  This  question  is  probably  not  alto- 
gether separable  from  that  of  the  central  reflection 
of  binaural  interaction,  although  in  certain  contexts 
it  may  be.  For  this  reason,  discu.ssion  of  the  two  ques- 
tions will  overlap,  especially  from  the  anatomical 
point  of  view. 

The  crucial  anatomical  problem  in  any  theory  of 
bilateral  interaction  must  be  the  opportunity  for 
side-to-side  communication.  There  is  no  dearth  of 
such  opportunities  in  the  auditory  pathway.  Crossing 
occurs  at  the  trapezoid  body,  the  earliest  opportunity 
since  this  begins  at  the  level  of  entrance  of  the  cochlear 
nerves.  The  trapezoid  crossing  seems  to  provide  not 
only  the  first  but  also  the  most  essential  crossing  for 
maximum  representation,  both  quantitative  and 
qualitative,  of  the  left  ear  in  the  contralateral  hemi- 
sphere and  vice  versa  (5).  It  is  also  at  this  level 
that  the  superior  olivary  complex,  by  virtue  of  its 
bilateral  input  from  cochlear  nuclei,  provides  many 
of  the  ascending  fibers  of  the  lateral  lemniscus  which 
represent  either  ear.  Thus  the  basically  bilateral 
projection  of  this  system  has  its  anatomical  foundation 
almost  at  the  level  of  entrance  of  the  nerves. 

Fibers  of  the  lateral  lemniscus  cross  at  the  somewhat 
diffuse  commissure  of  Probst,  just  below  the  inferior 
colliculus.  No  functional  significance  of  this  commis- 
sure has   been   demonstrated   (5). 

A  third  opportunity  for  crossing  of  auditory  nerve 
fibers  occurs  at  the  commissure  of  the  inferior  collicu- 
lus. The  significance  of  this  crossing  appears  to  be 
largely  local,  carrying  fibers  from  colliculus  to  oppo- 
site colliculus  or,  at  most,  to  opposite  medial  genicu- 
late (i  1 1).  Its  function  with  respect  to  the  pathway  as 
a  whole,  as  measured  by  the  influence  of  its  presence 
or  absence  on  cortical  response  to  stimulation  of  the 
contralateral  ear,  seems  to  be  negligible  (5,  90). 

The  cortical  auditory  area,  like  other  cortical  areas, 
communicates  by  strong  connections  with  its  counter- 
part on  the  opposite  hemisphere  by  way  of  the  corpus 
callosum  (62;   also   Ades,    H.    \V..,    unpublished   ob- 


servations). The  functional  significance  of  this  with 
special  reference  to  audition  is  not  clear. 

A  problem  which  particularly  stimulated  some  of 
the  early  modern  research  in  audition,  and  on  which 
incidental  observations  have  since  been  made,  is 
that  of  the  bilateral  representation  of  each  cochlea. 
This  has  been  tested,  more  or  less  adequately,  in 
various  ways  (5,  13,  18,  45,  46,  63,  88,  108,  113). 
Functionally,  the  results  have  generally  indicated 
some  difference  but  usually  so  small  as  to  make  it 
difficult  to  detect  an  effect  on  acuity  of  even  the  de- 
struction of  one  ear.  Most  of  the  observations  on  cor- 
tical electrical  response  to  contralateral  versus  ipsi- 
lateral  and  bilateral  stimulation  of  the  ears  have 
revealed  some  small  difference  in  representation. 
Similar  results  have  been  differently  interpreted, 
apparently  depending  upon  the  point  of  view  of  the 
individual  investigator  more  than  on  any  other  fac- 
tor. One  could  sum  up  by  saying  the  difference  in 
representation  of  the  two  ears  at  one  cerebral  hemi- 
sphere is  often  statistically  different  Ijut  probably 
not   practically   different. 

In  a  more  ingenious  way,  however,  Rosenzweig 
(89)  has  succeeded  in  demonstrating  that,  while 
quantitatively  the  difference  in  the  effect  of  the  two 
ears  on  the  cortical  area  may  appear  nearly  equal 
when  one  ear  is  stimulated  at  a  time,  there  is  never- 
theless a  more  significant  difference  when  the  position 
of  the  stimulus  is  varied  with  respect  to  the  two  ears 
simultaneously  stimulated.  He  found  that  when  a 
sound  is  presented  at  one  side,  the  cortical  response 
s  greater  at  the  contralateral  than  the  ipsilateral 
hemisphere — the  farther  to  the  side,  the  greater  the 
difference.  When  the  sound  is  in  the  median  plane, 
the  cortical  activity  is  equal  at  the  two  hemispheres. 
Here  we  have,  then,  a  clear  correlation  between  audi- 
tory localization  and  differential  response  of  right 
and  left  cortical  auditory  areas.  We  know,  however, 
from  the  work  of  Neff  rf  al.  (67)  that  auditory  localiza- 
tion is  a  function  which  is  not  abolished  by  destruc- 
tion of  the  cortical  areas.  In  a  second  group  of  ex- 
periments, Rosenzweig  &  Wyers  C90)  found  some 
evidence  for  binaural  interaction  in  the  inferior 
colliculi,  although  not  of  the  same  kind  as  in  the  cor- 
tex. 

Other  than  the  studies  cited  briefly  in  the  foregoing, 
the  evidence  on  the  bilaterality  of  auditory  function 
is  relatively  scanty.  The  system  has  been  more  often 
than  not  treated  without  regard  for,  or  with  only 
incidental  attention  to,  this  structural  and  functional 
feature. 


CENTRAL   AUDITORY  MECHANISMS 


6X1 


Dispersion  of  Excitation,  Recurrent 
Pathways  and  Inhibition 

In  this  final  section,  the  author  would  like  to  take 
up  as  a  group  certain  considerations  which  have 
been  touched  upon  in  earlier  sections  of  this  chapter 
and  by  other  authors.  Presumably,  these  should  ul- 
timately apply  to  the  elucidation  of  central  auditory 
functions  but  are  at  present  speculativ-e  and  little 
supported  by  evidence.  They  must  for  the  present 
be  spoken  of  in  general  terms.  The  evidence  which 
will  be  cited  is  more  by  way  of  justifying  the  specula- 
tion than  of  supporting  a  theory;  indeed,  we  have  no 
complete  coherent  theory  to  offer  but  only  a  number 
of  facts,  some  connected,  some  possibly  connected, 
and  some  suggestions  of  things  which  should  be  con- 
sidered in  future  investigations. 

One  of  the  distinguishing  features  of  the  auditory 
system  is  the  multiplication  of  elements  at  succes- 
sively higher  nuclear  stations  in  the  pathway  (22). 
Coupled  with  this  are  the  twin  physiological  phe- 
nomena of  temporal  dispersion  of  electrical  response 
to  brief  (click)  stimuli  and  amplification  of  response 
(5,  25,  32,  36).  In  this  case,  amplification  takes  the 
form  of  increased  amplitude  of  response  and  tem- 
poral dispersion  of  extending  the  duration  of  response 
if,  for  example,  we  compare  these  factors  from  coch- 
lear nuclei  to  medial  geniculate  body.  The  simplest 
way  of  accounting  for  these  phenomena  is  by  assum- 
ing the  amplification  to  be  the  consequence  of  the 
larger  number  of  units  available  to  be  fired  in  the 
larger,  more  rostral  nucleus,  and  the  temporal  dis- 
persion to  be  the  consequence  of  the  multisynaptic 
connections  which  result  in  a  lateral  lemniscus  com- 
posed of  several  different  orders  of  fibers.  There  are 
additional  possibilities  which  might  contribute  to 
both  phenomena.  Each  nucleus  traversed  contains 
within  itself  the  neural  matrix  requisite  (quantitatively 
speaking)  to  an  internal  temporal  and  spatial  dis- 
persion process  in  addition  to  that  we  are  attributing 
to  the  pathway  as  a  whole.  The  inferior  colliculus 
constitutes,  in  one  sense  at  least,  a  tract  parallel  to 
the  lemniscal  fibers  which  pass  directly  to  the  medial 
geniculate;  lemniscal  fibers  diverge  to  the  colliculus 
and  fibers  from  it  reconverge  on  the  main  path  at 
the  medial  geniculate,  providing  a  possible  feed-back 
device  which  might  augment  the  dispersion  processes. 

We  also  know,  howe\'er,  from  many  electrophysio- 
logical studies  that  amplification  and  temporal  dis- 
persion are  only  two  of  the  things  which  may  happen 
to  a  burst  of  activitx'  in  the  auditorv  system  aroused 


by  so  simple  a  stimulus  as  a  click.  The  response  may 
also  be  reduced  in  amplitude  or  obliterated  by  pre- 
ceding or  simultaneous  auditory  stimuli  of  the  same 
or  different  type,  depending  upon  where,  when  and 
how  strong  the  two  are  relative  to  each  other  and  upon 
where  the  recording  is  done.  We  may,  at  this  point, 
recall  the  recurrent  fibers  mentioned  in  the  early 
description  of  the  pathway  as  still  another  feed- 
back portion  of  a  complex  input  to  a  given  nucleus. 
When  we  recall  that  stimulation  and  inhibition  are 
equally  possible  consequences  of  the  firing  of  one 
neuron  into  another  (depending  on  conditions  at  a 
given  instant),  we  must  realize  the  term  "feed  back' 
as  used  here  may  be  positive  or  negative  with  respect 
to  the  whole  or  a  part  of  the  pattern  being  processed 
in  a  given  nucleus  at  a  given  time. 

To  make  concrete  the  rapidly  growing  po.ssibilities 
in  the  foregoing  paragraphs,  there  is  evidence,  cited 
by  Galambos  (32)  in  a  discussion  of  inhibition,  that 
inhibition,  intranuclear  or  that  provided  by  recur- 
rent fibers  or  both,  may  in  the  cochlear  nuclei  act 
to  restrict  the  frequency  range  to  which  a  given  unit 
responds.  He  further  indicates  that  other  studies  are 
in  progress,  both  anatomical  and  physiological  which 
we  can  hope  will  lead  to  an  expansion  of  this  factual 
nucleus. 

We  are  not  constrained  to  limit  the  possibilities  of 
selective  inhibition  or  other  forms  of  modulation  to 
the  elements  of  the  traditional  projection  pathway. 
It  has  already  been  pointed  out  in  discussing  the  retic- 
ular formation  that  the  auditory  system  feeds  in  some 
way  into  the  brain-stem  reticular  system  and  thereby 
exerts  its  influence,  in  common  with  other  sensory 
systems,  upon  the  status  of  general  cortical  activity 
quite  independently  or  indeed  in  the  absence  of  an 
intact  auditory  projection  pathway.  Adey  et  al.  (8) 
have  recently  demonstrated  also  that  various  cortical 
areas  (including,  as  it  happens,  A  I  in  the  cat)  fire 
freely  back  into  the  brain-stem  ascending  reticular 
system.  There  are  many  indirect  suggestions  in  the 
literature  which  would  lead  one  to  believe  that  this 
system  also  may  fire  into  the  specific  afTerent  path- 
ways. This  would  provide  a  reciprocal  arrangement 
at  brain-stem  levels  which  would  permit  interaction 
not  only,  in  this  ca.se,  between  the  auditory  system 
and  the  cortex  via  the  reticular  system  but  also  be- 
tween the  auditory  and  other  sensory  systems. 

Any,  all  or  none  of  the  foregoing  connections  may 
exist  and  work  in  any,  all  or  none  of  the  ways  sug- 
gested. But  one  thing  is  clear,  namely  that  the  central 


6l2 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


nuclei  of  the  auditory  system  must  operate  on  a  plan 
which  is  far  more  complex  than  the  simple  projection 
pathway  in  terms  of  which  we  often  speak.  There  is  no 
reasonable  doubt  that  each  nucleus  processes  the 
information  it  receives  from  an  input,  complex  both 
as  to  sources  and  patterns,  in  relation  to  events  which 
have  occurred  or  are  occurrins;  in  centers  above  and 


below,  auditory  and  nonauditory.  It  seems  most 
unlikely  that  substantially  further  progress  will  be 
made  toward  explaining  the  facts  of  audition  in 
neurophysiological  terms  without  considering  the 
intrinsic  and  extrinsic  neural  processes  by  which  ex- 
citation aroused  by  sound  is  modulated  in  the  central 
auditory  system. 


REFERENCES 


9- 

10. 


13- 

'4- 
1.5- 
i6. 

17- 


19. 

20. 


22. 
23- 
24- 

2,5- 

26. 

27- 
28. 

29- 
30 

31 

32 

33 


Ades,  H.  W.  a.  M.  a.  Arch.  Neurol.  &   Psychiat.  45:    138, 

1 941. 

Ades,  H.  \V.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  59,  1943. 

Ades,  H.  W.  J.  Neurophysiol.  7;  415,  1944. 

Ades,  H.  W.  J.  Neuropath,  &  Exper.  Neurol.  5:  60,  1946. 

Ades,  H.  W.  and  J.  M.  Brookhart.  J.  Neurophysiol.   13: 

189,  1950- 

Ades,  H.  VV.   and  R.   E.   Felder.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  49, 

1942- 

Ades,  H.  W.  and  D.  H.  Raab.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   12:   loi, 

■949- 

Adev,  W.  R.,  J.  P.  Segundo  and  R.  B.  Livingston.  J. 

Neurophysiol.  20;  i,  1957. 

Allen,  W.  F.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  144:  415,  1945. 

Aronson,  L.  R.  and  J.  W.  Papez.  .-1.  M.  A.  Arch.  Neurol. 

&  Psychiat.  32:  27,   1934. 

Bailev,   p.,   G.   von   Bonin,   H.   W.   G.^rol  .and  VV.   S. 

McCuLLOCH.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  6:  122,  1943. 

Bard,  P.  and  D.  McK.  Rioch.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkim  Hosp. 

60:  73,  1937. 

Barnes,  W.  T.,  H.  W.  Magoun  and  S.  W.  Ranson.  J. 

Cornp.  Neurol.  79:  129,   1943. 

Bremer,  F.  Arch,  internal,  physiol.  53:  53,  1943. 

Bremer,  F.  Rev.  neurol.  87:  65,  1952. 

Bremer,  F.,  V.  Bonnet  .and  C.  Terzuolo.  Arch,  inlernat. 

physiol.  62  :  390,  1 954. 

Bremer,  F.  and  R.  S.  Dow.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  2:  308,  1939. 

Brogden,  \V.  J.,  E.  Girden,  F.  A.  Mettler  and  E.  A. 

Culler.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  1 16:  252,  1936. 

BusER,  P.  AND  G.  Heinze.  J.  phisiol.,  Paris  46:  284,  1954. 

Butler,   R.   A.,   I.   T.   Diamond  and  W.   D.   Neff.  J. 

Neurophysiol.  20:  108,   1957. 

Campbell,    A.    VV.    Histological   Studies   on    Localization    oj 

Cerebral  Function.  Cambridge:  Cambridge,  1905. 

Chow,  K.  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  95:  159,  1951. 

Clark,  W.  E.  LeGros.  J.  Anat.  70:  447,  1936. 

Diamond,  I.  T.  and  W.  D.  Neff.  In  press. 

Erulkar,  S.  D.,   J.   E.   Rose  and   P.  W.   Davies.   Bull. 

Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  99 :  55,  1 956. 

Evarts,  E.  V.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  435,  1952. 

Evarts,  E.  V.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  443,  1952. 

Fernandez,  O.  Laryngoscope  (>\  :  1152,  1951. 

Ferrier,  D.    The  Functions  of  the  Brain.  London :  Smitii- 

Elder,  1876. 

French,  J.    D.,    M.    Verzeano    and   H.    VV.    Magoun. 

A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  69:  505,  1953. 

Galambos,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  381,  1952. 

Galambos,  R.  Physiol.  Rev.  34;  497,  1954. 

Galambos,  R.  Fed.  Proc.  16;  43,  1957. 


34.  Galambos,  R.  and  H.  Davis.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  39,  1943 

35.  Galambos,  R.  and  H.  Davis.  Science  108:  513,  1948. 

36.  Galambos,  R.,  J.  E.  Rose,  R.  B.  Bromiley  and   J.  R. 
Hughes.  J.  Neurophysiol.  15:  359,  1952. 

37.  Girden,  E.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  55:  518,  1942. 

38.  Girden,  E.,  F.  .A.  Mettler,  G.  Finch  and  E.  \.  Culler. 
J.  Comp.  Psychol.  21  :  367-385,  1936. 

39.  Goldberg,  J.  M.,  L  T.  Diamond  and  VV.  D.  Neff.  Fed. 
Proc.  16:  47,  1957. 

40.  Gross,  N.  B.  and  VV.   R.  Tiiurlovi'.  J.  Neurophysiol.   14: 

409.  195L 

41.  Guild,  S.  R.  Ada  oto-lartng.  43:  199,  1953. 

42.  Hampson,  J.  L.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  12:  37,  1949. 

43.  Hamuy,  T.  P.,  R.  B.  Bromiley  and  C.  N.  VVoolsey.  J. 
Neurophysiol.  19:  485,  1956. 

44.  Hind,  J.  E.J.  .Neurophysiol.  16:  475,  1953. 

45.  Kemp,  E.  H.,  G.  E.  Coppee  and  E.  H.  Robinson.  Am.  J. 
Physiol.  120:  304,  1937. 

46.  Kemp,   E.  H.  and  E.  H.  Robinson.  .Im.  J.  Physiol.    120: 

316,  1937- 

47.  Kenned>  ,  T.  Doctor's  Thesis.  Cliicago:  Univ.  of  Chicago, 

'955- 

48.  KiANG,  N.  Am.  J.  Phisiol.  183:  635,  1955. 

49.  KiANG,  N.  Doctor's  Thesis.   Chicago:  Univ.  of  Chicago, 

■955- 

50.  KoRNMiJLLER,    A.    E.    Bioclektrische  Erscheinungen  der   Hirn- 
nndenjclder.  Leipzig:  Thiemc,  1937. 

51.  Kryter,  K.  D.  and  H.  VV.  .\des.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  56:  501, 

'943- 

52.  Larionow,  VV.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  76:  608,  1899. 

53.  Licklider,  J.   C.   R.  Doctor's  Thesis.   Rochester,  N.  Y. : 
Univ.  of  Rochester,  1941. 

54.  Licklider,  J.  C.  R.  .and  K.  D.  Kryter.  Fed.  Proc.  i  :  51, 
1942. 

55.  Lilly,  J.  C.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  176:  493,  1954. 

56.  Lilly',  J.  C.  and  R.  B.  Cherry.  J.  Neurophysiol.  17:  521, 

1954- 

57.  LiNDSLEY,  D.  B.,  L.  H.  Schreiner,  VV.  B.  Knowles  and 
H.  VV.  Magoun.  Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  2 : 

483.  I950- 

58.  LiPMAN,  E.  A.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  62:  215,  1949. 

59.  Lorente  de  No,  R.  Laryngoscope  43 ;  i ,  1 933. 

60.  Lorente  de  N6,  R.  Tr.  Am.  Otol.  Soc.  27;  86,  1937. 

61 .  Magoun,  H.  VV.  A.  .\l.  .-1.  Arch.  .Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  67  :  145, 
1952- 

62.  Mettler,  F.  .\.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  55;  139,  1932. 

63.  Mettler,  F.  A.,  G.  Finch,  E.  Girden  and  E.  A.  Culler. 
Brain  57:  475,  1934. 


CENTRAL    AUDITORY    MECHANISMS 


613 


64.  Meyer,  D.  R.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  J.  Neurophysiol.   15:  89. 

149.  195^- 

65.  MicKLE,  W.   A.   AND  H.   VV.   Ades.   Am.  J.   P/iysiol.    170:  90. 
682,  1952. 

66.  Neff,  W.  D.  J.  Comp,  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  40:  203,  1947.  91. 

67.  Neff,  W.  D.,  J.  F.  Fisher,  I.  T.  Diamond  and  M.  Yela.  92. 
J.  Neurophvsiol.  19:  500,  1956. 

68.  NiEMER,  W.  T.   AND  S.   K.   Cheng.  Anal.  Rec.    103:  490,  93. 

'949- 

69.  Papez,  J.  \V.  A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychial.  24:  i,  1930.  94. 

70.  Perl,   E.   R.   and  J.   U.   C.asby.  J.  Nmrophysiol.    17:  429, 

1954-  95- 

71.  PoLIAK,  S.  J.  Anai.  60:  465,   1926. 

72.  PoLlAK,  S.    The  Main  AJjerent  Fiber  Systems  of  the  Cerebral  96. 
Cortex  in  Primates.  Berkeley:  Univ.  California  Press,  1932. 

73.  Pribram,  K.   H.,  B.  S.   Rosner  and  VV.  A.  Ro.senblith.  97. 
J.  Neurophysiol.  17;  336,  1954. 

74.  Raab,  D.  H.  and  H.  W.  Ades.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  59:  59,  1946.  98. 

75.  Ram(5n    y    Cajal,    S.    Histologie    due    Systeme    Nerveux    de 

I' Homme  et  des  Vertebres.  Paris:  Maloine,  1909,  vol.  I.  99. 

76.  Ramon  y  Cajal,  S.  Histoloi^ie  du  Systeme  Nerveux  de  I'Homme 

el  des  Vertebres.  Paris:  Maloine,   1911,  vol.  II.  100. 

77.  Rasmussen,  a.  T.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  63:  501,  1936.  loi. 

78.  Rasmussen,  G.  L.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  84:  141,  1946.  102. 

79.  Rasmussen,  G.  L.  Anat.  Rec.  106:  69,  1953. 

80.  Rasmussen,  G.  L.  J.  Comp.  .Neiirol.  99:  61,  1953.  103. 

81.  Riopelle,  a.  J.  AND  H.  W.  Ades.  J.  genet.  Psychol.  83:  63,  104. 

'953- 

82.  Riopelle,  A.  J.,  H.  F.  Harlow,  P.  H.  Settlage  and  105. 
H.  W.  Ades.  J.  Comp.  &  Physiol.  Psychol.  44:  283,  1951.  106. 

83.  Rose,  J.  E.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  91  :  409,  1949.  107. 

84.  Rose,  J.  E.,  R.  Galambos  and  J.  R.  Hughes.  Anal.  Rec.  108. 

'27:58,  1957- 

85.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  J.  Comp.  .Neurol.  91  :  441,  109. 
1949.  no. 

86.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  Cortical  Connections  and 
Functional  Organization  of  the  Thalamic  Auditory  System  oj  the  1 1 1. 
Cat.  Univ.  of  Wisconsin  Symposium,  Madison,  Wisconsin, 
August,  1955.  To  be  published.  112. 

87.  Rosenzweig,  M.  Am.  J.  Psychol.  59:  127,  1946.  113. 

88.  Rosenzweig,  M.  .{m.J.  Physiol.  167:  147,  1951. 


Rosenzweig,   M.   J.   Comp.   &   Physiol.    Psychol.    47:   269, 

'954- 

Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  and  E.  |.  Wyers.  J.  Comp.  £?  Physiol. 
Ps\chol.  48:  426,  1955. 

ScHUKNECHT,  H.  F.  Ttuns.  Am.  Acad.  Ophth.  57:  366,  1953. 
ScHUKNECHT,  H.  F.  AND  W.  D.  Neff.  Ada  oto-laryng.  42: 
263,  1952. 

ScHUKNECHT,  H.  F.  AND  S.  SuTTON.  .4.  M.  .4.  .Arch.  Oto- 
laryng.  57:  129,  1953. 

Snider,  R.  S.  Office  of  Naval  Research,  Monthly  Research 
Report,  Aug.   1930,  p.   15. 

Snider,  R.  S.  and  A.  Stowell.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  7:  331, 
1944. 

Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  W.  Taylor  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.   14:461,   1951. 

Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  W.  Taylor  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  14:479,   1951. 

Stevens,  S.  S.,  J.  G.  C.  Lorinc  and  D.  Cohen  (editors). 
Bibliography  on  Hearing.  Cambridge:  Harsard,   1955. 
SuMi,  T.,  Y.  Katsuki  and  H.  Uchiyama.  Proc.  Jap.  .Acad. 
32:  67,  1956. 

Tasaki,  I.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  17:  97,  1954. 
T.^s.-^Ki,  I.  AND  H.  Davis.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18:  151,  1955. 
Thurlow,  W.   R.,   N.  B.   Gross,   E.   H.   Kemp  and   K. 
LowY.  J.  Neurophysiol.,  14:  289,  1951. 
Tunturi,  a.  R.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.  144:  389,  1945. 
Vogt,  O.  Compt.  rend.  hebd.  scan,  et  Mem.  Soc.  de  biol.    10: 

54.  1898. 
Walker,  A.  E.  J.  .Anal.  72:  319,  1937. 
Waller,  E.  H.  J.  .Anal.  74:  528,  1939. 
Walzl,  E.  M.  Laryngoscope  57:  778,  1947. 
Walzl,  E.  M.  and  C.  N.  Woolsey.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hasp.  79:  309,   1946. 

Wever,  E.  G.  Theory  of  Hearing.  New  York:  Wiley,  1949. 
Woollard,  H.  H.  and  J.  A.  Harpman.  J.  Neurol.  & 
Psychial.  2:35,  1939. 

Woollard,  H.  H.  .'\nd  J.  .\.  Harpman.  J.  .Anal.  74:  441, 
1940. 

Woolsey,  C.  N.  Fed.  Proc.  6:  437,  1947. 
Woolsey,  C.  N.  and  E.  M.  Walzl.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hosp.  71:  315,  1942. 


CHAPTER    XXV 


Vision — introduction 


H.    K.    HARTLINE     |     Rockefeller  Institute  for  Medical  Research,  New  York  City 


WE   ARE   INDEED  'CHILDREN   OF  THE  SUN'.   The  ultimate 

dependence  of  li\ing  organisms  on  solar  enersy  is 
probably  one  reason  why  animals  came  to  evolve 
highly  specialized  sensory  receptors  for  exploiting  the 
sun's  radiations.  And  since  the  green  plants  utilize 
wavelengths  in  that  part  of  the  solar  spectrum  reach- 
ing the  earth's  surface  in  greatest  amount,  it  is  not 
surprising  that  the  receptors  evolved  by  plant-eating 
animals  and  their  predators  also  should  operate  in 
roughly  the  same  range  of  wavelengths,  which  we  in 
consequence  call  visible  light. 

Wavelengths  of  visible  light  are  small  compared 
with  the  size  of  the  bodies  of  most  animals  and  of 
many  significant  objects  in  their  surroundings.  Hence 
light  reflected,  scattered  and  absorbed  in  varying 
degrees  by  objects  in  an  animal's  environment  makes 
an  ideal  physical  agent  for  providing  information 
about  that  environment.  This  possibility  has  been 
exploited  by  nearly  all  animal  forms  to  a  remarkable 
degree. 

It  is  appropriate  that  a  neurophysiologist  taking  up 
the  study  of  vision  should  begin  with  a  consideration 
of  the  extraordinary  diversity  in  which  eyes  have 
evolved  in  'lower'  animal  forms.  An  intimation  of 
this  diversity  is  given  in  the  Milnes'  chapter  on  in- 
vertebrate photoreceptors.  Missing  from  this  hand- 
book is  a  comparable  discussion  of  the  eyes  of  the 
vertebrates,  which,  though  of  but  a  single  type, 
nevertheless  show  a  great  variety  of  ingenious  adap- 
tations to  meet  special  needs.  Fortunately,  this  de- 
ficiency is  easily  remedied  by  reference  to  Walls' 
excellent   and    highly   readable   hook.    The    Vertebrate 

EyeCn')- 

Of  the  great  variety  of  visual  organs  that  the  animal 
kingdom  has  developed,  many  are  no  mean  per- 
formers. Our  own  eyes,  for  all  their  defects,  are 
excellent  physical  instruments,  all  the  more  remark- 


able for  being  constructed,  by  embryological  magic, 
out  of  gristle  and  jelly.  Yet  man  need  not  think  he 
has  the  best  of  all  possible  eyes.  He  terms  the  short 
wavelengths  'ultraviolet',  but  they  are  visible  to  at 
least  some  insects.  Polarized  light  elicits  the  entoptic 
phenomenon  known  as  '  Haidinger's  brushes',  the 
orientation  of  which  reveals  the  direction  of  the 
plane  of  the  light's  polarization.  A  few  individuals 
are  said  to  be  able  to  perceive  these  brushes  when 
viewing  the  blue  sky  with  unaided  vision.  But  as  far 
as  is  known  to  the  author,  no  race  of  men  has  utilized 
this  as  a  sky  compass,  comparable  to  the  use  made 
by  many  of  the  arthropods  of  their  ability  to  sense 
the  plane  of  polarization  of  sky  light.  Man's  eyes  are 
remarkably  sensitive;  they  can  detect  approximately 
loo  quanta,  but  many  nocturnal  vertebrates  un- 
doubtedly have  a  lower  effective  threshold.  Our 
visual  acuity  is  surpassed  by  that  of  some  other  ani- 
mals, especially  the  acuity  of  birds  of  prey.  Yet  for 
all  of  this,  man  is  at  no  very  great  disadvantage 
merelv  because  the  visual  apparatus  of  other  animals 
surpasses  his  own  in  some  special  directions.  His 
visual  equipment  is  not  over-specialized,  and  it  does 
many  things  very  well. 

Interest  in  light  and  vision  dates  back  to  antiquity. 
Nearly  everyone  has  heard  of  the  quaint  idea  of  the 
Greeks,  that  light  is  an  intangible  ray-like  emanation 
from  the  eye  itself,  exploring  tactually  the  surround- 
ings. (Indeed,  if  we  were  to  assume  that  sensation 
could  result  only  if  such  emanation  were  absorbed  by 
what  we  term  luminous  objects,  this  idea  would  not 
be  easy  to  disprove;  in  physics  the  optical  principle 
of  the  reversibility  of  path  is  often  invoked  in  theo- 
retical discussions.)  With  a  history  of  many  interesting 
misconceptions,  a  sound  understanding  of  the  nature 
of  light  and  the  structure  and  function  of  the  eye 
gradually  emerged.  By  the  time  of  Kepler  many  of 


615 


6i6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  essentials  of  physiological  optics  were  beginning 
to  be  clear.  In  the  first  few  chapters  of  his  book,  The 
Retina,  Polyak  (ii)  summarized  in  scholarly  and 
interesting  style  the  early  history  of  this  subject,  from 
the  Greeks  and  Arabs  through  Medieval  to  Modern 
times. 

As  the  science  of  optics  developed,  it  took  two 
paths.  On  the  one  hand,  the  physics  of  light  emerged. 
Optics  through  most  of  its  history  depended  ulti- 
mately on  visual  observations  made  by  the  human 
eye  as  the  final  detecting  and  measuring  instrument. 
Only  relatively  recently  have  physicists  been  able  to 
replace  human  vision  to  ad\antage  by  the  photo- 
graphic plate  and  by  elaborate  photoelectric  detecting 
and  recording  devices.  The  laws  of  reflection  and  re- 
fraction were  first  derived  by  simple  visual  operations, 
conducted  in  a  scientific  manner.  This  history  is 
discussed  in  some  detail  in  a  recent  paper  by  Ratliff 
(13).  Combined  with  the  lens  maker's  art,  the  physics 
of  lenses  and  mirrors  developed  into  our  present  day 
geometrical  optics.  Physical  optics  is  based  on  the 
observation  of  the  maxima  and  minima  detected  by 
the  eye  in  interference  and  diffraction  patterns,  and 
by  brightness  changes  produced  by  polarization  op- 
tics. Photometry  was,  and  still  is  to  some  extent, 
dependent  on  the  ability  of  a  human  observer  acting 
as  a  null  device  to  detect  very  small  inequalities  in 
brightness  in  an  illuminated  field.  Even  color  vision, 
properly  a  subject  belonging  to  physiology,  has 
fascinated  physicists  from  the  time  of  Newton,  when 
it  formed  the  basis  for  the  emerging  science  of  spec- 
troscopy. As  these  physical  sciences  developed,  they 
in  turn  were  applied  to  the  eye  itself,  and  physiologi- 
cal optics  resulted. 

Physiological  optics  had  its  great  flowering  in  the 
last  century,  with  the  epochal  work  of  v'on  Helm- 
holtz.  In  its  essentials  and  in  many  of  its  details,  the 
physics  of  the  dioptric  system  of  the  human  eye  was 
put  into  satisfactory  shape  by  Helmholtz,  and  is  em- 
bodied as  part  of  a  broad  study  of  visual  physiology 
in  his  three  monumental  volumes  Handbuch  der 
Physiologischen  Oplik  (16). 

Physiological  optics  is  by  no  means  a  finished  sub- 
ject, as  shown  plentifully  in  Fry's  chapter.  Even  the 
physics  of  the  eye,  narrowly  defined,  invites  creative 
effort  today.  In  a  broad  sense,  physiological  optics  is 
often  taken  to  include  most  of  visual  physiology. 
Perhaps  this  is  too  broad  a  definition,  but  it  is  wise 
to  avoid  drawing  arbitrary  boundaries  to  this  field. 

Photosensitivity,  that  essential  property  that  makes 
a  visual  organ  possible,  is  conferred  upon  the  special- 


ized receptor  cells  of  an  eye  by  their  possession  of 
certain  chemical  substances  that  can  absorb  light 
(and  therefore  are  pigments)  and  undergo  photo- 
chemical change.  This  reaction  must  be  such  as  to 
initiate  a  change  of  events  in  the  irritable  mechanism 
of  the  receptor,  leading  to  the  transmission  of  nervous 
influences  along  the  optic  pathwav. 

The  \isual  pigment  of  the  retinal  rods  of  the  verte- 
brate eye  was  discovered  by  Boll  and  carefully  in- 
\estigated  by  Kiihne  nearly  a  hundred  years  ago. 
The  essential  importance  of  '  visual  purple'  or  "  rho- 
dopsin'  in  \ision  was  questioned  for  many  years  be- 
cause of  two  misconceptions.  First,  it  was  argued 
that  since  no  such  pigment  could  be  observed  in  the 
cones,  none  was  there.  True,  the  pigment  of  the  cones 
is  different  from,  though  closely  related  to,  that  of 
the  rods,  and  it  is  more  diflicult  to  detect;  but  modern 
methods  are  adequate  for  its  detection  in  the  cones 
and  its  extraction  and  study  in  vitro.  The  second,  and 
less  obviously  fallacious  argument  was  that  the  visual 
purple  in  a  retina  bleached  on  exposure  to  light,  and 
yet  photosensitivity  remained.  It  was  not  realized 
that  the  restorative  processes  (already  described  by 
Kijhne)  would  operate  in  light  as  well  as  darkness, 
and  would  lead  to  a'stationary  state'  in  which  a  small 
but  significant  amount  of  visual  pigment  would  be 
present  in  the  receptor  for  indefinitely  long  periods. 
Even  in  bright  light,  an  active  turnover  of  visual  pig- 
ment, with  photolysis  and  regeneration,  takes  place 
continually,  and  photosensitivity,  while  reduced,  is 
still  present.  The  clear,  quantitative  formulation  of 
these  ideas  by  Hecht  in  his  classic  studies  of  the 
photosensory  mechanism  of  the  clam,  Mya,  opened 
a  new  era  of  visual  physiology.  Before  the  advent  of 
modern  biochemistry,  Hecht  applied  these  ideas  of 
photolysis,  regeneration  and  the  stationary  state  to 
ijasic  \'isual  phenomena  such  as  light  and  dark  adap- 
tation, inten.sity  discrimination  and  flicker.  The 
experiments  that  he  and  his  colleagues  performed 
using  animals  and  with  human  observers,  and  the 
theories  they  devised  to  explain  their  results,  still 
play  a  fruitful  role  in  the  field  of  visual  physiology  (9). 
But  by  now  it  has  become  clear  that  Hecht's  ideas, 
while  basically  sound,  were  oversimplified,  and  need 
to  be  reworked  in  the  light  of  more  recent  biochemical 
developments. 

At  the  present  time,  the  significance  of  visual  purple 
and  the  photosensitive  substances  related  to  it  is 
firmly  established.  The  biochemistry  of  these  visual 
pigments  is  one  of  the  most  actively  pursued  and 
most  exciting  topics  of  receptor  physiology,  as  amply 


VISION INTRODUCTION 


617 


demonstrated  in  Wald's  chapter.  The  pro£!;ress  that 
has  been  made  in  the  study  of  chemistry  of  the  primary 
photosensitive  substances  of  the  rods  and  cones  of  the 
vertebrate  retina,  and  the  receptors  of  a  few  inverte- 
brates, is  indeed  impressive. 

It  was  a  significant  step  when  the  visual  pigments 
could  be  extracted  from  their  loci  in  the  outer  limbs 
of  the  rods  and  cones,  and  bleached  and  resynthe- 
sized  in  vitro.  Another  important  step  has  now  been 
taken  by  Rushton  and  his  colleagues  (14),  who  have 
succeeded  in  measuring  the  bleaching  and  regenera- 
tion of  visual  pigments  of  both  rods  and  cones  in  the 
living  eye,  as  described  in  Wald's  chapter.  Operating 
on  the  principle  of  the  ophthalmoscope,  a  sensitive 
photoelectric  device  is  used  to  measure  the  light  re- 
flected back  through  the  retina  of  a  human  subject. 
Rushton's  studies  are  providing  a  link  between  the 
biochemical  knowledge  of  the  visual  pigments,  and 
the  physiology  of  the  living  retinal  receptors. 

Biochemistry  alone  is  not  sufficient  to  solve  the 
problem  of  the  photoreceptor.  In  the  living  eye, 
visual  pigments  are  part  of  highly  organized  cellular 
systems.  New  concepts  of  the  fine  structure  of  visual 
receptor  cells  are  emerging  from  recent  cytological 
investigations.  In  the  developing  vertebrate  retina, 
the  rods  and  cones  originate  as  ciliated  epithelial 
cells  from  the  neural  tube.  [This  subject  has  been 
reviewed  by  Detwiler  (2)  and  by  Walls  (17).]  The 
cilia  becoine  transformed  (i)  into  the  outer  segments, 
which  are  long  stacks  of  doui^le-membrane  disks 
(15).  Remnants  of  the  original  ciliary  structure  re- 
main visible  to  electron  microscopy  in  the  com- 
pletely developed  receptors  (i,  12).  In  arthropods,  the 
osmium-staining  '  membranes'  take  the  form  of 
densely  packed  microvilli  of  the  surfaces  of  the  retinula 
cells,  so  that  the  rhabdom  has  a  structure  resembling 
a  honey-comb  (4,  10,  18).  Rhodopsin  is  present  only 
in  the  outer  segments  of  the  rods,  and,  as  Wald 
points  out  in  his  chapter,  constitutes  a  large  fraction 
of  their  bulk.  Prcsumaijly  a  similar  arrangement  of 
visual  pigment  holds  for  the  cone  outer  segment 
and  for  the  invertebrate  rhabdomere  as  well.  These 
cytological  facts  will  have  to  be  taken  into  considera- 
tion in  any  theory  of  the  receptor  mechanism. 

.•\  photoreceptor  is  a  transducer  of  light  energy 
into  nervous  action.  The  first  step,  the  photochemical 
change  in  a  specific  visual  pigment,  is  now  quite 
familiar.  The  later  steps,  ultimately  resulting  in 
nervous  excitation  that  is  transmitted  in  the  afferent 
nerve  fibers,  are  almost  completely  unknown.  Wald 
and  Granit  in  their  chapters  have  indicated  some  of 


the  possibilities  that  are  to  be  considered  [see  also 
(5)  and  (7)].  Presumably  at  least  some  of  these  proc- 
esses in  the  photoreceptor  are  not  basically  different 
from  those  occurring  in  any  other  cell  of  the  nervous 
svstem.  Indeed,  it  would  not  be  surprising  if  the 
entire  photosensitive  mechanism  were  the  result  of 
but  a  comparatively  minor  modification  of  a  funda- 
mental irritable  structure  of  a  cell.  The  photosensi- 
tivitv  of  some  ganglion  cells,  as  discussed  in  the 
Milnes'  chapter,  and  the  fact  that  peripheral  nerves 
can  be  photosensitized  by  dyes  (3)  makes  this  a  not 
unreasonable  expectation. 

The  final  outcome  of  the  excitatory  processes  ini- 
tiated by  light  is  the  generation  of  trains  of  nerve 
impulses  in  the  fibers  of  the  optic  pathway.  Whether 
all  photoreceptor  cells  themselves — the  rods  and 
cones  in  the  vertebrate  retina,  the  retinula  cells  in 
the  arthropod  compound  eye,  for  example — actually 
generate  trains  of  discrete  impulses  in  their  own  fibers 
is  not  established;  but  some  primary  receptor  cells  do, 
and  so  do  neurons  closely  associated  with  the  recep- 
tors. Optic  nerve  fiber  activity  consists  of  the  rhyth- 
mic succession  of  propagated  '  all-or-none'  disturb- 
ances typical  of  the  activity  of  all  neurons  concerned 
with  transmitting  influences  rapidly  over  large  dis- 
tances. Studies  of  the  discharge  of  impulses  in  single 
optic  nerve  fibers  have  shown  that  many  of  the 
familiar  phenomena  of  vision  have  their  origin  in 
properties  of  the  receptors,  or  of  the  retinal  neu- 
rons (6). 

Intimately  associated  with  the  excitation  of  the 
visual  mechanism  are  comparatively  slow  electrical 
changes  measurable  grossly  as  the  retinal  action  po- 
tentials. These  are  discussed  in  Granit's  chapter.  As 
a  result  of  studies  employing  microelectrodes  that 
are  small  enough  in  some  instances  to  penetrate 
single  cells  and  record  electrical  activity  from  within 
them,  the  .significance  of  various  components  of  the 
retinal  action  potentials  is  gradually  becoming  clearer. 
It  seems  likely  that  an  integral  link  in  the  excitatory 
process  is  a  change  in  electrical  polarization  of  cellu- 
lar structures,  brought  aijout  somehow  by  the  photo- 
chemical system  of  the  receptor.  As  in  other  parts  of 
the  nervous  system,  these  electrical  changes,  because 
of  the  local  current  flow  they  engender,  result  in  the 
initiation  of  relaxation  oscillations  in  cellular  mem- 
branes which,  conducted,  are  the  trains  of  nerve 
impulses  that  constitute  the  sensory  message  to  the 
higher  centers. 

An  eye  is  more  than  a  simple  mosaic  of  photore- 
ceptor elements.  The  histological  complexity  of  the 


6i8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


vertebrate  retina  and  of  the  compound  eye  of  insects 
is  ample  evidence  that,  in  the  more  highly  developed 
eyes,  sensory  information  from  the  transducer  ele- 
ments is  acted  upon  almost  immediately  by  highly 
organized  ganglionic  structures.  The  physiological 
studies  that  bear  out  this  expectation  are  reviewed  in 
Granit's  chapter,  where  the  conception  of  the  retina 
as  a  nervous  center  is  thoroughly  developed.  In  the 
vertebrate  retina,  excitatory  and  inhibitory  influences 
spread  and  converge,  and  interplay  in  a  complex 
manner  to  generate  patterns  of  optic  nerve  activity 
that  are  much  more  than  mere  copies  of  the  patterns 
of  light  and  shade  on  the  receptor  mosaic.  Even  in 
more  primitive  eyes,  simple  interactions  of  receptor 
units  take  place  (8)  that  serve  to  accentuate  certain 
significant  features  of  the  stimulus  pattern,  at  the 
expense  of  exact  fidelity  of  reproduction.  Integrative 
nervous  processes  begin  \ery  early  indeed  in  the 
visual  pathway.  More  than  this,  Granit's  chapter 
reveals  that  the  visual  receptor  organ,  like  other 
sense  organs,  is  under  a  certain  amount  of  centrifugal 
control  from  the  higher  nervous  centers.  This  new 
development  in  neurophysiology  is  already  having 
far-reaching  effects  on  ovn-  understanding  of  sensory 
physiology. 

With  the  study  of  the  physiology  of  the  higher  visual 
centers  of  the  brain,  taken  up  in  Hartley's  chapter, 
visual  physiology  merges  with  other  branches  of 
neurophysiology.  In  this  area,  contributions  come 
from  workers  not  primarily  concerned  with  vision, 
for  brain  physiology  involves  the  integration  of  all 
forms  of  neural  activity  that  govern  the  organism's 
behavior.  Quite  properly,  many  references  to  the 
physiology  of  the  visual  centers  will  be  found  scat- 
tered in  other  chapters  throughout  this  work. 

In  the  analysis  of  central  nervous  system  mecha- 
nisms, extensive  u.se  has  been  made  of  experimental 
animals  in  which  parts  of  the  brain  have  been  ab- 
lated, fiber  pathways  interrupted  or  specific  areas 
stimulated  artificially.  The  resulting  modifications  of 
behavior   then   reveal   important   physiological   rela- 


tionships. Applied  to  the  visual  system,  such  studies 
require  more  than  a  casual  familiarity  with  specific 
principles  of  retinal  physiology  and  with  overall 
visual  performance.  This  is  brought  out  in  Hartley's 
chapter.  Especially  in  the  field  of  animal  behavior  it 
should  be  emphasized  that  great  advances  have  been 
made  recently  by  experimental  psychologists.  Ani- 
mal behavior  can  now  be  controlled  more  effecti\ely 
and  studied  with  greater  precision  than  was  possible 
only  a  few  years  ago.  The  present  day  neurophysiolo- 
gist  must  master  these  powerful  new  techniques  or 
work  in  close  collaboration  with  colleagues  who  have 
mastered  them. 

The  aim  of  the  studies  that  have  been  outlined 
above  and  taken  up  in  detail  in  the  chapters  that 
follow  is  to  understand  vision.  This  broad  aim  can 
be  expressed  quite  explicitly,  so  far  as  many  ol  the 
behavioral  manifestations  of  vision  are  concerned. 
The  reactions  of  intact  animals  to  stimulation  by 
light  and  the  reports  of  human  observers  in  response 
to  visual  presentations  have  been  studied  by  truly 
scientific  methods  for  many  years.  Experimental 
psychology  provides  a  vast  amount  of  very  detailed 
and  very  precise  information  about  just  how  animals 
do  react,  what  human  subjects  do  report,  in  carefully 
controlled  visual  experiments.  Students  of  visual 
phenomena  have  not  neglected  the  analysis  of  their 
observations  in  attempts,  often  very  successful,  to 
provide  an  understanding  of  underlying  mechanisms. 
Indeed,  many  such  mechanisms  are  now  being  veri- 
fied by  direct  neurophysiological  experimentation. 
Psychological  studies  of  vision  are  vitally  important 
to  the  visual  neurophysiologist,  for  it  is  this  field  of 
science  that  sets  many  of  his  ultimate  problems.  No 
matter  how  far  we  may  progress  in  the  analysis  of 
the  neurophysiological  mechanisms  of  the  visual 
pathway,  our  task  of  acquiring  .scientific  understand- 
ing will  not  be  complete  without  the  complementary 
act  of  synthesizing  our  detailed  knowledge  into  a 
coherent  whole. 


REFERENCES 

1.  DeRobertis,   E.  J.   Biophyi.  &   Biochem,   Cylol.   2,   .Supple- 
ment: 209,  1956. 

2.  Detwiler,    S.    R.     Vertebrate    Photoreceptors .    Experimental 
Biology  Monographs.  New  York:  MacMillan,   1943. 

3.  Fessard,    a.    Recherches   sur    I'Activite    Rytlmiique    des    NerJ 
holes.  Paris:  Hermann,  1936,  p.  130. 

4.  Goldsmith,  T.  H.  and  D.  Philpott.  J.  Biophys.  &  Bio- 
chem. Cylol.  3:  429,   1957. 

5.  Granit,   R.    Receptors  and  Sensory   Perception.    New    Haven; 
Yale  Univ.  Press,  1955. 


6.  Hartline,  H.  K.  Harvey  Lectures  Ser.  37:  39,  1942. 

7.  Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  E.  F.  MacNichol, 
Jr.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Qiiant.  Biol.   17:  125,  1952. 

8.  Hartline,   H.   K.,   H.   G.   Wagner   and   F.   Ratliff.  J. 
Gen.  Physiol.  39 :  65 1 ,   1 956. 

9.  Hecht,  S.  Physiol.  Rev.  17:  239,  1937. 

10.  Miller,  W.  H.  J.  Biophys.  &  Biochem.  Cytol.  3:  421,  1957. 

11.  PoLVAK,  S.  L.   The  Retina.  Chicago:  Univ.  Chicago  Press, 

194"  • 


VISION — INTRODUCTION 


619 


12.  Porter,  K.  R.  H an n  Lectures  fict.  51:  175,  1957. 

13.  Ratliff,  F.    In:   Psychology:   A   Study   oj  a   Science  (vol.  4), 
edited  by  S.  Koch.  New  York:  McGraw  Hill.  In  press. 

14.  RusHTON,  W.  A.  H.  AND  F.  W.  Campbell.  Nature,  London 
174:  1096,  1954. 

15.  SjoSTRAND,  F.  S.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  42:  15,   1953. 

16.  VON    Helmholtz,    H.    Handbuch    der    Physiologischen    Optik, 


III  Auflage.  Hamburg  and  Leipzig:  L.  Voss,  1909.  (Eng- 
lish transation,  J.  P.  C.  Southall  (editor).  Menasha,  Wis- 
consin: Banta,  1924- 1925.) 

17.  \Vali5,  G.  L.   The  Vertebrate  Eye  and  its  Adaptive  Radiation. 
Bloomfield  Hills,  Michigan:  Cranbrook  Press,   1942. 

18.  WoLKEN,  J.  J.,  J.  Capenos  AND  A.  TuRANO.  J.  Biophys.  & 
Biochem.  Cytol.  3;  441,  1957. 


CHAPTER    XXVI 


Photosensitivity  in  invertebrates' 


LOR  us  J.   MILNE2 
MARGERY   M  I  L  N  E^ 


Durham,  .h'eiv  Hampshire 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Photosensitivity  in  Unicellular  Organisms 
Cells  Without  Obvious  Photoreceptors 
Cells  With  Obvious  Photoreceptors 
Photosensitivity  in  Multicellular  Organisms 

Photosensitivity  Mediated  Without  Obvious  Receptors 

Ganglionic  photosensitivity 

Peripheral  photosensitivity 
Photosensitivity  Mediated  Through  Unicellular   Eyespots 
Photosensitivity  Mediated  Through  Multicellular  Eyes 

Compound  eyespots 

Ocelli  or  simple  eyes 

Compound  ocelli  or  aggregate  eyes 

Stemmata 

Compound  eyes 

Camera-style  eyes  in  moliusks 

Camera-style  eyes  in  annelids 

Camera-style  eyes  in  arthropods 
Phenomena  Related  to  Stimulus  Intensity 
Pigment  Migration  Within  the  Eye 
Spectral  Sensitivity  and  Color  Vision 
Form  Perception  and  Pattern  Recognition 


EYE-MINDED  MAN  IS  proDC  to  forgct  that  the  fundamen- 
tal irritability  of  protoplasm  includes  a  sensitivity  to 
radiant  energy  in  the  spectral  region  he  knows  as 
light.  An  eye  is  a  specialization  with  which  a  multi- 

'  Contribution  from  the  Scripps  Institution  of  Oceanography, 
La  Jolla,  California,  New  Series  No.  967.  The  information  in 
this  chapter  has  been  assembled  with  the  aid  of  research  grants 
from  the  .American  Academy  of  .\rts  and  Sciences,  the  Ameri- 
can Philosophical  Society,  the  E.xplorers  Club  and  the  Society 
of  the  Sigma  Xi. 

'  Professor  of  Zoology,  University  of  New  Hampshire. 

^  Recently  Visiting  Professor  of  Biology,  Northeastern  Uni- 
versity, Boston,  Mass. 


cellular  animal  may  gain  additional  information  from 
a  light  stimulus.  Usually  it  is  a  device  allowing  a 
central  nervous  system  to  be  better  informed  about 
events  in  the  surrounding  environment.  An  eye  im- 
plies a  nervous  mechanism  of  some  kind,  but  the 
converse  is  not  true. 

In  photosensitivity  the  initial  event  is  absorption 
of  photons — the  quanta  of  radiant  energy — by  some 
substance  which  is  altered  by  this  addition.  When 
compared  to  thermal  reactions,  most  photochemical 
changes  appear  to  be  in  a  class  by  themselves  charac- 
terized by  temperature  coefficients  so  small  that  they 
are  described  as  'temperature-independent.'  Through 
the  temperature  range  within  which  living  things  are 
active  this  is  correct  enough. 

Since  only  the  absorbed  energy  is  effective  in  pro- 
ducing a  photochemical  change,  every  photosensitive 
mechanism  must  contain  a  chemical  substance  which 
can  trap  photons.  When  the  concentration  of  such  a 
substance  is  high  enough  so  that  a  few  per  cent  of 
incident  photons  are  absorlied,  we  may  detect  the 
absorption  as  an  opacity  and  recognize  the  absorbing 
substance  as  a  pigment. 

So  far  none  of  the  pigments  found  to  be  responsible 
for  photosen.sitivity  in  living  systems  are  neutral  in 
their  absorption.  They  are  not  gray  but  colored  be- 
cause they  absorb  most  at  one  wavelength  and  less 
at  others.  This  feature  determines  the  spectral  sensi- 
tivity of  the  system. 

For  an  animal  to  be  informed  continuously  regard- 
ing the  radiant  energy  reaching  its  surface,  it  must 
be  able  to  produce  continuously  the  pigment  which 
is  altered  by  the  absorption  of  photons.  In  the  dark 
this  production  would  be  expected  to  decrease  in 
rate  until  the  pigment  reached  a  maximum  concen- 


621 


622 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


tration.  Simultaneously  the  mechanism  would  attain 
its  maximum  sensitivity  to  light  energy.  In  continuous 
illumination  the  system  should  reach  an  cquiliijrium 
such  that  the  rate  at  which  the  pigment  is  altered  by 
absorbed  energy  is  equal  to  the  rate  at  which  the 
pigment  is  produced.  The  time  required  to  reach 
maximum  concentration  in  the  dark  has  been  found 
to  be  several  times  as  great  as  that  required  to  reach 
an  equilibrium  in  continuous  illumination.  The 
former  is  a  measure  of  dark  adaptation  and  the  latter 
of  light  adaptation. 

Since  radiant  energy  arrives  a  quantum  at  a  time 
and,  according  to  Einstein's  law  of  photochemical 
equivalence,  is  absorbed  only  at  the  rate  of  one  quan- 
tum per  molecule  affected,  this  initial  step  in  photo- 
sensitivity has  a  statistical  character.  At  low  intensities 
of  light,  so  few  molecules  may  capture  a  photon  in  a 
given  time  that  the  organism  ignores  the  scattered 
events.  At  a  slightly  higher  intensity  of  stimulation, 
the  frequency  of  capture  would  rise.  If  the  lower 
limits  for  response  to  light  are  explored  with  a  test 
flash  of  constant  duration,  some  definite  intensity 
level  can  be  found  at  which  a  sensation  of  light  is 
obtained  50  per  cent  of  the  time.  At  a  slightly  lower 
intensity,  the  response  is  obtained  perhaps  30  per 
cent  of  the  time.  At  a  slightly  higher  intensity,  per- 
haps 80  per  cent  of  test  flashes  elicit  a  response.  Both 
subjective  and  objective  measurements  of  this  kind 
show  a  range  in  'frequency  of  seeing.'  Some  value, 
such  as  50  per  cent,  can  be  defined  as  threshold. 

Variation  in  response  at  threshold  may  be  entirely 
attributable  to  variations  in  the  quantum  content 
of  test  flashes.  Whether  one  molecule  of  pigment 
modified  in  a  brief  time  (such  as  o.  i  sec.)  is  enough  to 
trigger  the  entire  photosensitive  mechanism  is  still 
unsettled  (10,  77,  97,  213).  Different  nervous  systems 
may  require  several  molecules  of  pigment  to  be  al- 
tered almost  simultaneously.  In  any  case  it  is  clear 
that  photosensitivity  has  an  efficiency  approaching 
the  theoretical  limit  of  one  quantum  and  one  mole- 
cule. 

Relatively  few  pigments  are  so  unstable  that  a 
single  photon  can  produce  a  chemical  change.  A 
photon  simply  lacks  the  amount  of  energy  required 
to  start  most  chemical  reactions.  From  this  it  might 
be  expected  that  photons  with  the  largest  content  of 
energy  would  be  most  important  in  photosensitivity. 
In  the  wavelength  band  visible  to  the  human  eye, 
that  giving  the  sensation  of  violet  consists  of  photons 
with  about  double  the  energy  of  those  in  the  red. 
Ultraviolet  includes  photons  with  an  energy  content 
double  that  of  photons  in  the  violet;  but  the  seem- 


ingly transparent  media  of  terrestrial  vertebrate  eyes 
ab.sorb  the  ultraviolet  before  it  reaches  the  photo- 
sensitive retina.  Aquatic  organisms  are  shielded  from 
ultraviolet  by  the  water  around  them.  Except  under 
laljoratory  conditions,  only  the  terrestrial  arthropods 
(such  as  insects)  appear  to  be  stimulated  visually  by 
wavelengths  shorter  than  400  m/x. 

The  photosensiti\e  pigments  extracted  from  in- 
vertebrate and  vertebrate  eyes  (152,  153,  276,  277) 
appear  consistent  in  having  their  eflTective  maximum 
of  absorption  between  400  and  700  m/z — well  within 
the  spectrum  visible  to  man  (fig.  i).  Indirect  evidence 
is  available  to  indicate  that  the  corresponding  pig- 
ment or  pigments  in  insects  may  be  more  affected  by 
the  ultraviolet  components  of  sunshine  than  by  energy 
absorbed  at  a  secondary  absorption  maximum  in  the 
human  range.  Hence  it  is  apparent  that  the  chemical 
adaptations  which  permit  photosensitivity  in  aquatic 
life  and  terrestrial  vertebrates  are  related  less  to  the 
energy  content  of  the  photons  than  to  the  wave- 
lengths of  radiant  energy  which  penetrate  most 
deeply  into  seas  (480  mju)  and  lakes  (560  m/ii).  Sensi- 
tivity to  ultraviolet  seems  to  have  come  secondarily 
as  a  gain  when  some  arthropods  became  both  ter- 
restrial and  diurnal. 

For  extraction  of  photosensitive  pigments  in  suf- 
ficient quantity  for  spectrophotometric  analysis,  con- 
siderable masses  of  photosensitive  tissue  are  needed. 
So  far  this  requirement  has  limited  direct  study  to  the 
large  eyes  of  squids  (20,  21,  65,  150,  229)  and  the 
stalked  eyes  of  euphausiid  crustaceans  (143)  which 
can  be  cut  from  hundreds  of  specimens  taken  with 
plankton  nets.  Most  other  invertebrates  are  either 
too  small  or  too  difficult  to  catch  in  adequate  num- 
bers for  a  biochemical  approach.  In  consequence 
other  avenues  of  investigation  have  been  necessary 
for  studying  their  photosensitivity. 

The  most  valid  approach  is  beset  with  technological 
difficulties.  It  consists  of  inserting  microelectrodes 
into  photosensitive  cells  and  recording  electrical  events 
which  follow  stimulation  of  the  cells  by  light.  These 
changes  in  electrical  potential  clearly  demonstrate 
the  peripheral  origin  of  nervous  activity  in  visual 
systems  (90)  and  suggest  that  depolarization  of  the 
photosensitive  cell  is  responsible  for  initiating  nerve 
impulses  in  its  associated  nerve  fiber  (177). 

With  some  invertebrate  eyes  it  is  possible  to  study 
impulses  in  surviving  nerve  fillers  emerging  from 
photosensitive  cells  (88,  89,  281,  286).  Far  easier  and 
more  widely  applicable  is  the  less  informative  pro- 
cedure of  applying  an  electrode  to  the  corneal  surface 
of  an  intact  eye  and  examining  the  gross  potential 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


623 


400       424 


491  500 


575  585  600 


648 


700       m^i 


ultra- 
violet 


vio- 
let 


blue 


green 


yei-f 

llow: 


orange 


red 


Infra- 
red 


410 


A 

A 

A 

A 

A 

463 

491 

522 

562 

620 

470 

520 

580 

600 

650 


FIG.  I.  The  absorption  maxima  of  extracted  and  synthesized  photosensory  pigments  range  across 
much  of  the  spectral  range  visible  to  man.  Commonly  accepted  boundaries  (jop)  and  representative 
centers  {holtom)  of  appropriate  wavelengths  of  light  arc  shown  for  each  hue  sensation.  Photosensory 
pigments  include:  463,  euphausiopsin  (Kampa,  1955);  491,  rhodopsin  (Kiihne,  1877)  and  cephalop- 
sin  (Bliss,  1948);  5.^2,  porphyropsin  (VVald,  1937);  5^.2,  iodopsin  (Wald,  1937);  and  620,  cyanopsin 
(Wald,  1953). 


changes  which  accompany  illumination  of  the  organ 
(6,  7,  87).  Excised  surviving  eyes  can  be  studied  in 
the  same  way  although  without  gaining  from  them 
any  additional  information  (56,  211,  212). 

Far  more  unknowns  arc  encountered  in  trying  to 
learn  about  an  animal's  photosensitivity  from  its  be- 
havior either  under  laboratory  conditions  or  undis- 
turbed in  its  natural  habitat.  Yet  the  vast  bulk  of 
physiological  investigations  on  invertebrate  vision 
employ  methods  of  this  type.  In  them  one  advantage 
can  be  seen :  the  reactions  of  the  whole  animal — even 
in  an  artificial  environment — must  be  closer  to  its 
responses  in  normal  life.  By  observing  behavior,  some- 
thing more  of  the  role  of  vision  in  ordinary  situations 
can  be  gathered.  Isolated  measurements  of  electrical 
potentials  are  far  more  difficult  to  interpret  on  an 
ecological  basis. 

By  far  the  most  hazardous  approach  to  photosensi- 
tivity in  animals  is  also  the  commonest.  It  is  decep- 
tively easy  to  examine  their  photosensory  structures 
anatomically  and  histologically  and  to  infer  how  these 
structures  may  be  used.  Valuable  evidence  can  cer- 
tainly be  obtained  as  to  limitations  imposed  by  struc- 
ture; but  without  careful  experimentation  with  li\ing 
individuals,  there  is  no  way  to  be  sure  that  the  aniinal 
exploits  its  photo.sensory  mechanism  in  its  daily  life. 

In  most  groups  of  in\'ertebrates  the  best  that  can 
be  done  in  summarizing  findings  on  photosensitivity 
is  to  relate  the  anatomical  and  behavior  studies.  This 


is  approached  most  simply  on  a  structural  basis  or  on 
a  taxonomic  framework  (31,  77,  142,  193,  216,  269). 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    UNICELLULAR    ORGANISMS 

Since  both  receptor  and  eff"ector  are  component 
parts  of  the  same  cell  in  protozoans,  photosensory 
specializations  are  more  limited  than  among  meta- 
zoans.  Responses  to  light  seem  correspondingly  re- 
stricted to  movements  of  the  whole  cell  or  of  its  loco- 
motory  structures,  such  as  flagella. 

Cells  Without  Obvious  Photoreceptors 

There  is  no  a  priori  reason  to  assume  that  the  re- 
sponses to  light  found  in  amebas  need  correspond  to 
those  in  such  flagellates  as  Peranema.  In  the  former,  an 
increase  in  intensity  of  illumination  is  usually  fol- 
lowed by  retraction  of  pseudopodia.  The  rate  of 
locomotion  of  amebas  appears  to  be  afifected  signifi- 
cantly by  the  intensity  of  continued  illumination. 
Initially  the  rate  is  modified  by  the  state  of  dark 
adaptation  of  the  cell  (183).  Hertel,  who  investigated 
the  ultraviolet  to  280  m//  as  a  stimulus  (104),  postu- 
lated that  the  radiations  catalyzed  the  release  of 
hydrogen  peroxide  within  the  cell  and  that  these 
chemical  changes  accounted  for  behavior.  Mast  & 
Stahler  (183)  believed,  instead,  that  the  light  pro- 
duced a  physical  change  in  the  elastic  strength  of  the 
plasmagel,  inhibiting  the  formation  of  pseudopodia. 


624 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


In  Peranema  the  whole  cell,  including  its  flagellum, 
appears  to  he  sensitive  to  light  (238,  239).  A  gradient 
can  be  detected  from  a  minimum  in  the  posterior  end 
to  a  maximum  in  the  flagellum.  Even  detached  flagella 
will  respond  to  increased  illumination  by  bending, 
but  no  recovery  seems  possible.  Hence  both  the  re- 
ceptor and  effector  substances  must  be  widely  dis- 
tributed in  the  protoplasm,  but  the  recovery  phase 
depends  upon  transport  of  additional  materials  into 
the  flagellum  from  the  cell  body  where  they  are 
elaborated. 


Cells  With  Obvious  Pholoreceplors 

Definite  organelles  called  stigmata  are  present  in 
many  flagellates  and  seem  associated  with  a  localized 
photosensitivity.  Some  stigmata  are  ball-like  masses 
of  opaque  red  or  black  pigment.  This  is  the  case  in 
Euglena  where  the  stigma  is  close  to  the  double  base 
of  a  single  flagellum  and  must  shade  the  flagellar 
bases  from  radiations  reaching  the  cell  from  straight 
ahead.  If  it  is  assumed  that  shading  allows  the  flagel- 
lar mechanisms  to  operate  at  full  speed  and  illumina- 
tion from  the  side  inhibits  the  lashing  movement,  then 
the  polarity  of  swimming  movements  with  respect  to 
a  point  source  can  be  explained  rather  simply. 

Cup-shaped  and  spoon-shaped  stigmata  are  usual 
among  colonial  flagellates  such  as  Gonium  and  Volvox. 
The  concavity  of  the  stigma  is  associated  with  the 
hypersensitive  protoplasm  and  may  be  lined  with  a 
reflecting  layer  which  serves  as  a  concave  mirror  and 
concentrates  the  light  at  a  focal  point  with  the  photo- 
sensitive region.  Both  Gonium  and  Volvox  stigmata 
possess  a  lens  as  well.  In  Volvox  the  size  of  the  stig- 
mata decreases  with  distance  of  the  cell  from  the  an- 
terior pole  of  the  colony,  and  all  stigmata  are  placed 
so  as  to  face  outward  and  slightly  toward  the  an- 
terior pole.  The  two  flagella  of  each  cell  beat  in  dif- 
ferent modes  and  at  unlike  rates  according  to  the 
direction  from  which  light  reaches  the  stigma.  Mast 
worked  out  the  paths  of  the  reflected  and  refracted 
rays  (180,  182)  but  did  not  identify  the  functional 
connection  between  the  photosensitive  mass  in  the 
stigmatic  area  and  the  locomotor  mechanism  at  the 
flagellar  bases.  It  is  clear,  however,  that  when  a  Volvox 
colony  is  illuminated  only  from  directly  ahead  on  its 
axis  of  symmetry,  every  cell  receiving  radiations  does 
so  in  ways  which  lead  to  symmetrical  beating  of  the 
flagella.  Under  these  circumstances  the  entire  colony 
revolves  on  its  axis  and,  unless  the  light  intensity  is 
excessive,  approaches  the  source  while  so  rotating. 
Unilateral    illumination,     by    contrast,     appears    to 


modify  flagellar  movements  on  the  illuminated  side 
while  vigorous  beating  on  the  shaded  side  gradually 
turns  the  colony  until  its  axis  is  directed  toward  the 
source. 

Mast  (182)  presented  generalizations  concerning 
the  form  and  function  of  stigmata  in  unicellular  and 
colonial  flagellates,  without  mentioning  the  most  re- 
markable of  them  all.  In  Pouchetia  and  related  dino- 
flagellates,  the  lens  associated  with  the  stigma  is 
enormous  and  spherical.  The  resemblance  to  a  multi- 
cellular eye  in  these  unicellular  organisms  is  striking. 
No  experimental  work  has  been  reported  which 
might  show  the  use  to  which  Pouchetia  puts  this 
striking  organelle. 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    MULTICELLUL.'^R    ORG.ANISMS 

With  multicellularity  a  metazoan  might  be  ex- 
pected to  show  pronounced  localization  of  photo- 
sensitivity into  obvious  eyes.  Some  metazoans  manage 
quite  well  and  respond  to  light  without  obvious 
specializations  of  this  kind.  Others,  although  equipped 
with  eyes,  seem  to  ignore  visual  cues  for  considerable 
parts  of  their  life  histories. 

Photosensitivity  Mediated  Without  Obvious  Receptors 

To  this  phenomenon  the  phrases  'dermoptic  sense' 
and  "dermal  photosensitivity'  have  often  been  ap- 
plied (e.g.  133-135,  201).  Table  i  indicates  the 
taxonomic  groups  in  which  a  generalized  response  of 
this  kind  has  been  demonstrated.  Often  a  failure  to 
recognize  the  presence  of  this  photosensory  system  in 
animals  with  eyes  has  led  to  wrong  conclusions  con- 
cerning the  effects  of  unilateral  blinding. 

Ganglionic  Photosensitivity 

In  1934,  Welsh  (290)  and  Prosser  (218)  discovered 
independently  that  the  abdominal  ganglia  of  the 
crayfish  were  photosensitive,  permitting  the  animal 
to  respond  to  light  even  after  its  eyes  had  beeti  re- 
moved. Hess  (113)  found  the  same  sensitivity  in 
abdominal  ganglia  of  the  shrimp  Crangon  and  the 
spiny  lobster  Panulirus  but  learned  that  photosensory 
cells  were  scattered  along  nerves  in  such  remote 
parts  of  the  body  as  the  uropods.  The  role  of  gangli- 
onic photosensitivity  in  controlling  locomotor  ac- 
tivity of  the  intact  animal  has  received  some  con- 
sideration (232).  Probably  it  is  more  important  at 
the  time  of  molt,  before  the  new  exoskeleton  has  de- 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


62  = 


TABLE   I.  Structural  and  Functional  Aspects  of  Photoreceptors  as  Presently  Known 
in  the  Various  Taxnnomic  Groups  of  Invfrlehrales 


Reoresentatlon 

a  =  all 
1    =  larval 
m  =  most 
s   =  some 
v  =  noted 

0  > 

S  > 

—    tn 

fi 

Stigmata  (Intracellular 

organelles)   also 
General  photosensitivity 
Neuronal  photoreceptors 
Unicellular  eyespots 
Compound  eyespots 
Ocelli   =  simple  eyes 
Compound  ocelli 
Stemmata 

Compound  eyes  (ommatldia) 
Camera-style  eyes 
Retina  direct 
Retina  inverted 

Pigment-cell  Iris  diaphragm 
Muscular  iris  diaphragm 
Tapetum 

Migratory  eye  pigments 
Muscular  shift  of  lens 
Hydraulic  shift  of  retina 
Muscular  shift  of  retina 
Muscular  reshaping  of  lens 
'  Ladder  retina  ' 
Binocular  field 
Color  vision 

PROTISTA 

Mastigophora 

S 

Sarcodlna 

a 

Cillata 

a 

COELENTERATA 

Hydrozoa 

V        s        s                     m  s 

Scyphozoa 

V                 m  8                  a 

Anthozoa 

V 

CTENOPHORA 

V 

PLATYHELMDJTHES 

Turbellarla 

V        m        s                       s  n 

1                                          s 

Trematoda 

V         s 

NEMERTINEA 

V                  s                       s  n 

1 

ASCHELMINTHES 

Rotlfera 

V       m        s                     a 

Gastrotrlcha 

V       m       s                     a 

Kinorhyncha 

V        m 

Nematoda 

V                 s                     a 

_ 

BRYOZOA 

V 

MOLLUSCA 

Amphlneura 

V                  m                      a 

Gastropoda 

V                 s                 s    m  E 

8                   S     8    S     8 

Pelecypoda 

V   V   1    s    s                 s   m  ( 

i                  S                                 8           S 

Cephalopoda 

V                                   a   a 

am                    8      ? 

ANNELIDA 

Archiannellda 

V        m 

Polychaeta 

V        s    s    s                 8  m  s 

i                               B                               B 

Ollgochaeta 

V      V 

Hlrudlnea 

V        m 

CHAETOGNATHA 

V                       m 

1 

TARDIGRADA 

V        m 

OhreCHOPHORA 

V                  a                       a 

AHTHROPODA 

U 

Branchiopoda 

V                  m           m        a 

m       8                                    s 

Ostracoda 

m           ma 

m       8 

Copepoda 

m           m  8    a 

m       8 

Branchlura 

m           ma 

m 

Clrrlpedia 

m           1          a 

a 

Malacostraca 

V   V             s             ma 

ass                         s     8 

Trllobita 

m            m         ? 

? 

Xlphosura 

a            a        a 

8 

Eurypterlda 

m           m        ? 

? 

■g 

Scorpionida 

V                 m                     a 

S 

Pseudoscorplonlda 

V                 m                     a 

8 

Phalanglda 

V                  m 

a            s 

Acarlda 

V                 8                     a 

Araneida 

V                 m                    m 

8                  8                   S 

Solpuglda 

8                     a 

8 

Pycnogonlda 

m 

a 

Chllopoda 

V                  m            8        m 

8     8 

Dlplopoda 

V                       8     8                      m 

8 

t 
1 

Ametabola 

V                 s           m        a 

S 

Hemlmetabola 

V                 8            m       a 

m       s                              s 

Holometabola 

1         1         8    8  8    m       a 

m       a    8                         8     8 

ECHINODERMATA 

Crlnoldea 

V 

Holothuroldea 

V     V 

Asteroldea 

V                         8     8 

Echinoldea 

V 

Ophluroldea 

V 

HEMICHORDATA 

V     V     1 

CHORDATA 

Urochordata 

V     V                 T 

a 

Cephalochordata 

V        a 

626 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


veloped  its  pigmentation  and  while  light  can  pene- 
trate more  readily  into  the  viscera. 


Peripheral  Photosensitivity 

The  degree  to  which  light  responses  can  be  localized 
into  effective  behavior  patterns  without  structural 
specialization  of  photoreceptors  is  certainly  shown  in 
the  echinoid  echinoderms.  With  the  Mediterranean 
urchin  Centrosteplianiis,  von  Uxkiill  found  fairly  rapid 
adjustments  in  orientation  of  the  spines  according  to 
the  direction  from  which  the  body  was  shadowed 
(275).  A  more  detailed  study  of  the  Caribbean 
Diadema  by  Millott  (i 88-1 91)  confirmed  von  Uxkiill's 
findings  on  behavior  and  established  the  fact  that  no 
special  photoreceptor  cells  are  present  at  the  ends  of 
the  twigs  from  the  radial  nerves  where  these  enter 
the  dermis.  Yet  the  entire  body  surface  appears 
photosensitive,  only  the  spines  themselves  lacking  this 
type  of  irritability.  The  radial  nerve  must  be  intact 
for  the  responses  to  follow  local  stimulation.  Even  a 
kind  of  dark  adaptation  is  present  through  concentra- 
tion of  pigment  in  dermal  chroma tophores,  permit- 
ting more  light  to  penetrate  to  the  level  in  the  skin  at 
which  the  nerve  twigs  lie. 

With  another  Caribbean  urchin  Millott  was  able 
to  duplicate  some  of  the  spectacular  findings  of  Dubois 
(59)  on  European  Strongylocentrotus.  Both  echinoids 
have  the  habit  of  partially  covering  the  aboral  sur- 
face with  debris  picked  up  from  the  adjacent  bottom. 
The  Caribbean  Lvtechinus  inhabits  the  coral  reefs  and 
boulder-strewn  beaches  to  the  limit  of  wave  action 
at  low  tide  and  appears  to  use  bits  of  coral  as  ballast 
in  this  buffetted  zone;  but  if  a  narrow  beam  of  light 
is  directed  on  any  portion  of  the  aboral  surface,  the 
urchin  transfers  these  opaque  objects  (or  any  bits  of 
seaweed  within  reach  of  tube  feet  and  pedicellariae) 
into  the  path  of  the  beam,  using  them  as  a  parasol. 
Urchins  at  greater  depths,  where  the  light  is  less  in- 
tense, seem  to  carry  debris  only  while  sunlight  is 
reaching  them  on   the   bottom  (191). 

Perhaps  the  best  example  of  an  extreme  sensitivity 
to  shadows  was  found  by  Millott  with  Diadema.  When 
a  single  urchin  was  placed  in  a  finger  bowl  of  sea 
water  under  a  checkerboard  of  electric  lamps,  it 
would  rapidly  point  many  spines  in  the  direction  of 
any  single  lamp  in  the  pattern  when  this  one  was 
temporarily  turned  off.  Identification  of  the  direction 
in  which  so  minor  a  change  occurred  must  be  medi- 
ated through  an  inherent  polarity  with  maximum 
sensitivity  to  light  reaching  the  body  surface  at  right 


angles,  as  well  as  through  the  general  roundness  such 
as  Nagel  postulated  (201)  (fig.  2  right~). 

A  general  photosensitivity  with  less  striking  re- 
sponses has  Ijeen  demonstrated  in  other  echinoderms: 
in  the  entire  aboral  surface  of  the  sessile  (and  swim- 
ming) crinoid  Antedon  (163);  over  the  whole  body  of 
the  holothurians  Synaptula  (203)  and  Holothuria  (41); 
o\er  the  aboral  surface  of  asteroids  from  which  the 
ocellatc  tips  of  the  arms  had  been  removed  (258,  297); 
and  in  the  ophiuroid  Ophiocoma  (37).  Crozier  found 
a  difference  (41)  between  the  behavior  of  Holothuria 
and  Thyone  in  that  the  latter  holothurian  mo\cd  away 
from  a  light  source  as  an  echinoid  might — any  angle 
of  the  body  in  advance.  Holothuria,  by  contrast, 
showed  a  functional  polarity,  swinging  around  until 
the  mouth  was  farthest  from  the  stimulating  light 
before  moving  off  in  this  orientation. 

Responses  to  light  where  no  receptors  seemed 
specialized  toward  sensiti\ity  to  radiations  have  been 
reported  in  blinded  and  intact  members  of  many 
phyla:  in  the  hydrozoan  medusa  Gonionemus  (200);  in 
luminescent  ctenophores  Beroe  (122)  and  Mnemiopsis 
(197);  in  blind  turbellarians  (165);  in  blind  rotifers 
(263,  264);  in  nematodes  (104);  in  oligochaetes  (no, 
III,  114,  161)  with  identification  of  neuronal  photo- 
receptors in  which  photo.sensitivity  was  localized;  in 
the  polychaete  Mercierella  (228);  in  the  leech  Hirudo 
(234);  in  bryozoans,  both  as  larvae  (Pectinatella)  and 
adults  QLophopus'),  through  kinetic  responses  of  nega- 


FIG.  2.  Curvature  of  the  body  surface  can  provide  an  animal 
having  general  photosensitivity  with  a  means  for  identifying 
the  direction  from  which  a  light  stimulus  comes.  Neither  lamp 
A  nor  shadow  B  have  a  directional  significance  for  a  flat  photo- 
sensitive tissue  (/f/O;  but  in  a  cylindrical  or  spherical  organism 
(jigtiO  quite  different  cells  are  illuminated  by  the  two  sources, 
A  and  B.  [After  Nagel;  from  Milne  &  Milne  (193).] 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


627 


live  sign  (179);  in  various  pelecypods  (201)  but 
particularly  Mya  (94,  96,  148,  166);  in  the  gastropod 
Helix  on  the  basis  of  photosensitivity  in  spite  of  a  gap 
found  in  the  optic  nerve  from  the  tentacular  eye 
(306);  in  the  hemichordate  Dolichoglossus  (39,  11 1, 
112);  and  in  the  adult  urochordates  Ascidia  (93)  and 
Ciona  C96). 

Photoreceptors  in  the  skin  of  a  soft-bodied  animal, 
such  as  an  oligochaete,  vary  greatly  in  degree  of  ex- 
posure depending  on  the  extension  and  contraction 
of  the  superficial  tissues  in  locomotion.  In  Perichaeta, 
Harper  (85)  found  that  a  negative  response  might  be 
shown  to  low-intensity  illumination  w-hen  the  worm 
was  tested  while  extended,  yet  the  same  test  applied 
while  the  worm  was  contracted  might  lead  to  a  posi- 
tive response. 

Quite  a  few  eyeless  invertebrates  (particularly 
hydroid  coelentcrates  and  turbellarians)  react  posi- 
tively to  sunlight  without  giving  proof  that  they  are 
themselves  photosensitive.  These  organisms  harbor 
mutualistic  algae  (green  'zoochlorellae'  or  brownish 
green  'zooxanthellae')  which  carry  on  photosynthesis 
when  illuminated.  A  response  to  chemical  changes 
accompanying  photosynthesis  could  account  for  the 
behavior  of  the  animal  partner  in  many  instances. 
Even  when  the  invertebrate  possesses  eyes  as  well  as 
mutualistic  algae,  the  role  of  vision  in  photic  re- 
sponses becomes  suspect  until  pro\ed  definitely. 

Translucent  bodies  which  stud  the  exposable  por- 
tion of  the  mantle  in  the  giant  clam  Tridacna  were 
described  as  eyes  until  Yonge  (305)  cleared  up  the 
misunderstanding.  He  found  that  these  structures  are, 
instead,  an  adaptation  permitting  daylight  to  reach 
deep  levels  of  the  mantle  tissue  where  large  numbers 
of  mutualistic  algae  grow.  Tridacna  appears  to  depend 
for  food  primarily  upon  the  success  of  the  enclosed 
algae.  It  raises  the  plants  in  mantle  greenhouses,  and 
the  supposed  eyes  are  merely  illuminators  in  the  roof! 

Photosensitivity  Mediated  Thrmigh  Unicellular  Eyespots 

Addition  of  a  cup  of  opaque  pigment  beside  a 
photosensory  cell  seems  but  a  small  step  in  evolution 
but  still  a  move  toward  development  of  an  eye.  This 
addition  permits  the  receptor  to  be  more  definite  as 
to  the  direction  from  which  a  stimulating  light  comes 
than  when  its  own  greater  sensitivity  to  radiation 
passing  along  its  axis  is  the  sole  means  for  differentia- 
tion. When  photosensory  inechanisms  consist  of  a 
single  receptor  cell  and  an  associated  pigment  mass, 
the  term  eyespot  is  useful — although  earlier  authors 


have  used  the  word  far  more  loosely.  Often  a  lens  is 
associated  with  an  eyespot,  providing  still  more  dis- 
criminatory possibilities  and  perhaps  increasing  the 
structure's  sensitivity  by  gathering  in  more  light. 

Eyespots  are  present  in  such  turbellarians  as  Pro- 
rhynchus  (^14.4)  and  in  a  number  of  parasitic  trematodes, 
particularly  at  various  larval  stages  of  digenetic 
forms.  Some  trematode  cercaria  possess  them;  so  does 
the  miracidium  of  Fasciola.  Instances  of  apparent  de- 
generation have  been  identified  (70),  although  no 
evidence  has  been  given  that  would  indicate  a  cor- 
responding loss  of  sensitivity.  In  many  nemertineans, 
one  or  more  pairs  of  eyespots  are  present  (123),  but 
whether  negative  responses  to  radiation  found  in 
Lineus  (194,  195)  depend  upon  functional  eyespots 
has  not  been  proved.  By  ingenious  experiments, 
Viaud  has  been  able  to  distinguish  between  photo- 
sensitivity in  rotifers  mediated  by  their  eyespots  and 
those  elicited  on  the  basis  of  a  general  sensitivity 
(260-264).  Eyespots  are  the  only  photoreceptors 
identified  in  archiannelids  (125)  and  some  poly- 
chaetes  (i  17,  237).  They  are  characteristically  present 
in  tardigrades  (43)  and  larval  hemichordates  (243, 
245).  They  are  scattered  along  the  nerve  cord  of 
cephalochordates  (116,  141,  204),  and  appear  to  be 
the  sole  mechanism  allowing  response  to  radiations 
(204);  possibly  degenerate  eyespots,  devoid  of  pig- 
ment cups,  were  described  by  Joseph  (141). 

Pliotosensitii'ity  Mediated  Through  Multicellular  Eyes 

COMPOUND  EYESPOTS.  Another  small  step  toward  ef- 
fective vision  consists  of  the  grouping  together  of 
unicellular  eyespots,  forming  them  into  an  organized 
cluster  with  radially  divergent  axes.  Structures  of  this 
kind — compound  eyespots — have  been  reported  in  but 
three  groups  of  organisms  with  no  indication  that 
they  are  part  of  an  evolutionary  sequence. 

Both  solitary  and  compound  eyespots  project  from 
the  mantle  margin  of  pelecypods  in  the  genera  Area 
(fig.  6)  and  Pectunculus.  In  A.  noae  a  specimen  8.5 
cm  long  had  235  of  these  sensory  clusters.  Neither 
Patten  (206)  nor  Kiipfer  (159),  however,  indicated 
the  degree  to  which  the  compound  eyespots  were 
used  in  a  visual  way. 

The  annelids  Potamilla  and  Branchiomma  (fig.  6) 
bear  compound  eyespots  on  the  main  stems  of  the 
cephalic  branchiae  (3,  27).  Each  sensory  unit  is 
isolated  from  its  neighbors  by  pigment  cells.  Yet  the 
known  reactions  of  these  polychaete  worms  seem  no 


628 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


more  complex  than  those  of  other  genera  in  which 
the  structure  of  photoreceptors  is  simpler  (267). 

The  other  group  of  invertebrates  with  compound 
eyespots  conceals  its  photoreceptors  so  well  that  they 
were  unknown  before  1946.  Most  maggots  (larval 
flies,  insect  order  Diptera)  give  strong  negative  re- 
sponses to  any  but  very  dim  illumination,  and  many 
can  orient  themselves  with  remarkable  accuracy. 
Yet  it  remained  for  Bolwig  (22)  to  locate  the  photo- 
sensory  structures  using  microdissection.  He  found  a 
small  group  of  rounded  cells  somewhat  anterodorsal 
to  the  supraesophageal  ganglion.  In  early  first-instar 
lar\ae  these  cells  are  not  fully  developed;  neither  is 
photosen,sitivity.  By  the  second  instar,  the  cell  group 
is  well  organized  but  not  yet  surrounded  i^y  opaque 
tissues;  these  larvae  orient  well,  apparently  bv  dis- 
criminating the  faint  shadow  of  their  own  translucent 
bodies.  In  the  third  instar,  the  growth  of  the  pharyn- 
geal .skeleton  provides  an  opaque  cup  around  the 
compound  eyespot  without  blocking  the  light  from 
anterior  directions;  these  larvae  will  follow  the  vec- 
torial resultant  path  between  two  light  sources.  With 
later  development,  both  overgrowth  of  the  pharyn- 
geal skeleton  and  increased  opacity  of  the  body  re- 
duce the  accuracy  of  orientation  and  raise  the  thresh- 
old for  response. 

Eyes  of  types  other  than  compound  eyespots  agree 
in  having  a  layer  of  photosensory  cells,  i.e.  a  retina. 
It  may  line  a  pit  (fig.  3,  left")  or  lie  below  a  lens  (fig. 
3,  right).  Where  the  retina  consists  of  many  cells  so 
close  to  the  dioptric  elements  that  no  clear  image 
seems  possible,  the  term  ocellus  (simple  eye)  may  be 
applied.  If  a  similar  retina  is  remote  enough  from 
the  dioptric  system  that  a  reasonable  image  is  cast 
upon  it,  the  phrase  camera-style  eye  seems  preferable; 
camera-style  eyes  usually  have  an  accessory  mecha- 
nism permitting  accommodation.  If  the  retina  con- 
sists of  only  a  ring  of  receptor  cells,  clustered  around 
the  pro.ximal  end  of  the  dioptric  system  like  sections 
of  a  citrus  fruit,  the  structure  is  an  ommatidium. 
Ordinarily  ommatidia  are  grouped  into  a  compound 
eye  with  the  optic  axes  of  the  individual  units  diverg- 
ing from  one  another  on  a  quasiradial  plan.  Ocelli 
may  also  be  grouped  as  compound  ocelli,  or  'aggre- 
gate eyes.'  A  puzzling  intermediate  between  an  ocel- 
lus and  an  ommatidium  is  found  in  the  larvae  of 
some  holometabolous  insects;  for  this  structure  the 
word  stemma  is  useful. 

OCELLI  OR  SIMPLE  EYES.  OcelH  with  large  lenses  are 
located  around  the  rim  of  many  coelenterate  medusae, 
but  the  degree  to  which  their  photosensiti\ity  is  used 


—  PIGMENT    CELLS 


FIG.  3.  A  pigment-surrounded  cup  lined  with  photoreceptor 
cells  (/(>//),  a  cuticular  lens  above  the  retina  (jrighl'),  or  a  com- 
bination of  the  two,  are  characteristic  of  true  eyes.  .\  narrowing 
of  the  cup's  aperture  improves  the  ability  of  an  eye  to  dis- 
criminate between  events  at  A  and  B  but  reduces  the  amount 
of  light  admitted.  A  lens  provides  an  aid  to  discrimination  and 
also  can  collect  more  light,  hence  increasing  the  organ's  sensi- 
tivity. [After  Nagel;  from  Milne  &   Milne  (193).] 


to  modify  responses  arising  from  a  general  responsive- 
ness to  light  has  never  been  established  (e.g.  200). 
Nerve  fibers  from  these  eyes  communicate  with  the 
diffuse  nerve  net  and  may  be  part  of  a  much  more 
direct  .sensory-motor  mechanism  than  is  found  among 
animals  with  a  highly  developed  nervous  system. 

Shallow  and  deep  pigment  cups  without  lenses  are 
the  characteristic  ocelli  in  turbellarians.  Their  num- 
ber ranges  from  one  pair  to  many  and  their  size  from 
minute  to  relatively  large.  No  phylogenetic  pattern 
is  discernible,  and  no  correlation  has  been  made  with 
habits  or  habitat  (133).  Best  known  are  the  con- 
spicuous ocelli  of  Planaria  in  which  a  pigment  cup 
open  lateralis'  conceals  the  distal  ends  of  the  receptor 
cells  (fig.  4).  Hesse  (115)  related  the  visual  field  of 
each  ocellus  to  the  behaxior  of  the  intact  flatworm. 
Taliaferro  (249)  found  in  addition  that  receptors  in 
the  posterior  and  ventral  portions  of  each  pigment 
cup  arc  invoked  in  responses  wherein  the  animal 
turns  toward  the  eye  of  that  side,  whereas  stimulation 
of  the  remaining  receptor  cells  is  followed  by  a  turn 
in  the  opposite  direction. 

Amono  nemertineans  (123)  and  rotifers  (263,  264) 
the  presence  of  ocelli  rather  than  eyespots  has  been 
noted  in  se\eral  genera.  But  no  special  significance 
has  been  sttributed  to  the  more  complex  photosensory 
mechanism. 

So  wide  a  variation  in  ocellar  structure  is  present 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


629 


PLANAR  I A 


DORSAL 

EPITHELIUM 


MEDIAL 


( IN   CROSS    SECTION  ) 


FIG.  4.  The  dorsal  ocelli  of  the  turbellarian  flatvvorm  Planaiia  consist  of  opaque  pii^ment  cups 
open  laterally,  concealing  the  distal  ends  of  photoreceptor  cells.  Shadowing  of  the  photoreceptors 
by  the  pigment  cups  differs  in  horizontal  illumination  according  to  the  orientation  of  the  worm 
Qeft).  Each  receptor  cell  (right)  is  most  sensitive  to  radiations  passing  through  it  parallel  to  the 
long  axis  of  the  portion  within  the  pigment  cup.  [.After  Hesse;  from  Milne  &  Milne  (193).] 


among  the  many  eye-bearing  members  of  the  phyla 
Annelida,  Mollusca  and  Arthropoda  that  it  is  tempt- 
ing to  arrange  them  in  parallel  .series  (i  17,  193,  237). 
A  phylogenetic  basis  for  this  series  would  be  valuable 
(fig.  5),  Ijut  no  correlation  has  been  found  between 
form  of  ocelli  and  other  structural  features  or  with 
the  normal  habitats  occupied.  Hence  it  seems  prob- 
able that  the  variation  has  no  Ijroader  implications, 
and  the  parallels  in  embryonic  de\elopment  are 
fortuitous. 

The  several  paired  ocelli  on  the  prosiomium  of  the 
polychaete  Nereis  are  of  a  single  structural  type  with 
a  cuticular  lens  over  a  cupped  retina  (162,  199). 
The  most  anterior  pair  mav  mediate  negative  re- 
sponses to  light  and  the  others  positive  responses 
(105);  an  asymmetry  of  the  retina  in  the  anterior 
pair  appears  to  adapt  them  to  forward  and  lateral 
vision,  whereas  the  other  ocelli  are  directed  more 
vertically  upward  (199).  Brand  (24)  reported  that 
the  behavior  characteristic  of  unilaterally  blinded 
Nereis  is  shown  even  when  any  single  ocellus  is  left 
intact  on  the  operated  side. 

In  the  Atlantic  palolo  worm,  the  polychaete  Eunice, 
each  segment  bears  a  single  mid-ventral  ocellus,  but 
its  function  has  not  been  found  (in)  since  general 


photosensiti\ity  appears  to  account  for  responses 
observed. 

The  ocelli  of  leeches  appear  to  be  the  chief  special- 
ized sensory  organs  and  in  the  first  fi\e  body  .segments 
occupy  the  positions  corresponding  to  lateral-line 
organs  in  more  posterior  regions.  Each  ocellus  is  al- 
most cylindrical  with  its  longitudinal  axis  at  right 
angles  to  the  body  surface;  its  nerve  fibers  connect 
on  the  medial  surface.  Whether  they  are  phylogeneti- 
cally  related  to  tactile  elements  (293)  or  can  legiti- 
mately be  arranged  in  an  evolutionary  series  (266) 
has  not  been  proved.  Their  function  may  be  related 
more  to  body  pigment  distribution  (247)  than  to 
kinetic  responses.  As  Parker  pointed  out  (205),  mere 
possession  of  photoreceptors  does  not  imply  that  an 
animal  can  see. 

The  abundant  small  ocelli  of  amphineuran  mol- 
lusks  provide  a  comparable  puzzle.  An  adult  Cure- 
pliium  may  have  as  many  as  8500  of  these  structures, 
perhaps  3000  in  the  most  anterior  plate  of  the  shell 
(198,  214,  215).  Heath  (92)  traced  their  formation 
and  concluded  that  they  must  be  functional  even  in 
the  adult.  Crozier  (40)  could  find  only  a  general 
photosensitivity,  however,  in  Chiton.  It  was  most 
pronounced  in  the  scaly  girdle,  where  ocelli  are  lack- 


630  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


RANZANIDE5 


SYLLIS 


NEREIS 


PHYLLODOCE 


HAL/OTIS 


PATELLA 


SCUTUM 


SECRETION 


MUREX 


LIMULUS 


SCOLOPENDRA 


DYTISCUS    LARVA 


—  CUTICLE 


\ 

SAGITTA 
AND 
TRANSVER 
SECTIONS 


FIG.  5.  A  comparative  series  showing  degrees  of  development  in  ocelli  of  polychacte  annelids 
Qipper  roiv),  gastropod  moUusks  (^center  row)  and  arthropods  Qower  row),  the  ocelli  in  longitudinal 
section  in  all  instances.  No  phylogenetic  interpretation  seems  indicated.  [Limulus  after  Demoll, 
Scolopendra  after  Heymons,  Dyliscus  after  Giinther,  others  after  Hesse;  from  Milne  &  Milne  (193).] 


ing,  and  extended  over  the  soft  ventral  surface  of  the 
foot.  Moreover,  as  the  animal  aged,  its  response  to 
light  changed  from  negative  to  positive. 

Among  the  pelecypods,  Nagel  (201)  distinguished 
a  category  of  'ikonoptic'  organisms  in  which  the 
structure  of  the  ocelli  seemed  suitable  for  producing 
a  poor  image  in  the  receptor  cells.  Potamides  has  a 
single  layer  in  the  retina,  Pecten  (fig.  7,  left)  a  double 
layer;  in  both  instances  the  ends  of  the  receptor  cells 
are  turned  away  from  the  lens  so  that  the  retina  is 
inverted  (159,  209).  Wenrich  (291)  investigated  what 
he  believed  to  be  image-formation  in  Pecten  in  terms 
of  the  smallest  white  card  intensely  illuminated, 
movement  of  which  would  produce  a  shell-closing 
response  in  the  scallop.  A  more  probable  explanation 
for  his  observed  fact  is  that,  at  the  light  intensity 
used,  the  appearance  or  disappearance  of  the  card  in 
the  visual  field  furnished  the  minimum  effective  change 
in  brightness.  Hartline  (88)  found  by  electrical  means 


that  the  distal  (smaller)  layer  of  retina  mediates  a 
strong  off-response,  whereas  the  proximal  layer  dis- 
charges nerve  impulses  whenever  illuminated. 

A  remarkably  gradual  series  can  be  arranged  show- 
ing sectional  views  of  gastropod  eyes  (fig.  5,  second 
line),  but  the  significance  of  the  differences  noted  in 
terms  of  photic  responses  by  the  intact  animal  may 
be  questioned  (121,  216,  295).  The  sign  of  the  re- 
sponse appears  to  be  altered  by  many  other  factors, 
such  as  diet,  wetness  or  dryness  of  the  body  surface 
(196),  and  whether  the  animal  is  inverted  or  upright 
(74).  The  role  of  general  photosensitivity  in  these 
reactions  has  not  been  segregated  from  the  supposed 
dependence  upon  vision  through  the  ocelli. 

On  the  basis  of  embryonic  origins  and  neurologic 
connections,  Hanstrom  (82)  classified  ocelli  in  arthro- 
pods into  three  categories:  a)  the  nauplian  eyes  of 
crustaceans,  the  ocelli  of  insects,  the  median  eyes  of 
trilobites,   the  ocelli  of  xiphosurans  and  the  eyes  of 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


631 


pycnogonids — all  arising  from  a  dorsal  ectodermal 
mass  in  the  embryo;  6)  the  lateral  ("secondary") 
ocelli  of  modern  arachnoids  and  all  eyes  of  diplopods 
and  chilopods,  arising  through  degeneration  from 
the  ommatidia  of  compound  eyes  produced  by  the 
lateral  ectodermal  mass  of  the  embryo;  and  f)  the 
ventral  ocelli  of  trilobites  and  xiphosurans  and  the 
median  ("primary")  eyes  of  eurypterids  and  arachnids, 
arising  from  a  ventral  ectodermal  mass  in  the  embryo. 
No  clear  correlation  can  be  noted  between  these 
categories  and  the  detailed  anatomical  features  of 
the  ocelli  in  postembryonic  life — features  described 
and  illustrated  with  great  care  by  Grenacher  (81). 

In  many  planktonic  crustaceans  the  median  ocellus 
is  the  only  eye  present.  Fundamentally  it  appears  to 
be  a  double  structure,  but  fusion  may  be  remarkably 
complete.  Many  crustaceans  which  metamorphose 
lose  their  ocelli  as  they  grow.  An  extreme  example  is 
found  among  barnacles  (6g):  newly  hatched  nauplii 
have  a  bilobed  median  ocellus;  a  pair  of  compound 
eyes  appears  at  the  metanauplian  stage,  only  to  be 
extruded  or  to  degenerate  at  metamorphosis;  until 
this  time  the  median  ocellus  remains  unchanged,  but 
then  it  separates  into  two,  each  half  migrating  into  a 
lateral  position  and  continuing  as  the  sole  photo- 
sensory  specialization  of  the  adult. 

The  ventral  position  of  the  median  ocellus  in 
Branchipus,  Artemia  and  other  branchiopods,  many 
copepods,  some  trilobites  and  larval  xiphosurans  sug- 
gests that  inverted  swimming  may  be  an  ancestral 
habit.  Inverted  swimming  is  characteristic  of  Limulus, 
Branchipus  and  Artemia,  and  probably  was  also  of 
trilobites.  A  median  ocellus  must  be  of  help  while 
dorsal  compound  eyes  are  directed  toward  the  bot- 
tom rather  than  the  sky.  Persistent  nauplian  ocelli 
are  known  in  some  decapod  malacostracans.  In 
Artemia  the  ocelli  can  serve  alone  in  mediating  essen- 
tially all  normal  adult  responses  to  light  stimulation 
(171);  exceptions,  which  depend  upon  function  of 
the  compound  eyes,  are  the  visual  following  of  females 
by  males  and  a  convulsive  reflex  when  a  dark-adapted 
animal  is  suddenly  illuminated. 

Waterman  (281)  has  provided  a  convenient  table 
showing  the  groups  of  arthropods  in  which  a  median 
ocellus  is  known.  At  the  same  time  he  presented 
evidence  from  electrical  recordings  indicating  that 
messages  pass  along  the  optic  nerve  fibers  from 
Limulus  ocelli  comparable  to  those  from  the  com- 
pound eyes.  Their  use  by  the  animars  central  nervous 
system  remains  a  mystery. 

Eight  ocelli  or  less  are  characteristic  of  spiders 
(208);   the  arrangement   and   actual   number  varies 


from  one  genus  to  another.  One  pair,  the  'primary 
eyes,"  are  simpler  in  having  a  direct  retina  and  no 
tapetum,  although  the  entire  retina  may  be  moved 
within  the  body  through  the  contraction  of  paired 
muscles — perhaps  in  following  the  progress  of  prey 
or  potential  mate.  The  'secondary  eyes"  usually  have 
an  inverted  retina  and  often  a  tapetum;  no  move- 
ments of  the  retina  are  possible.  Nervous  connections 
to  the  two  types  of  ocelli  are  consistent  with  this 
difference  in  structure  and  with  Hanstr6m"s  generali- 
zations (82).  The  role  of  vision  is  difhcult  to  demon- 
strate (128-130,  217,  294)  in  spiders,  except  in  jump- 
ing spiders  (127).  These  have  been  recommended  as 
ideal  laboratory  material  because  they  seem  so  un- 
aware of  confinement. 

Among  centipedes  and  millipedes  which  have 
ocelli,  no  responses  to  light  ha\e  been  described 
which  could  not  be  accounted  for  adequately  on  the 
basis  of  a  general  photosensitivity  in  the  body  surface. 

Two,  or  at  most  three,  ocelli  are  present  in  many 
insects  (136,  137),  but  their  role  in  normal  living 
habits  has  been  a  puzzle  (23,  52,  53,  108,  118,  126, 
224).  When  only  the  ocelli  are  exposed,  insects  usu- 
ally behave  as  though  completely  blinded.  Some  show 
responses  which  cannot  be  accounted  lor  on  the  basis 
of  general  photosensitivity  (25).  Demoll  &  Scheuring 
(52)  found  considerable  correspondence  between  the 
visual  fields  of  the  compound  eyes  and  of  the  ocelli. 
This  discovery,  together  with  the  observation  that 
many  insects  with  their  ocelli  covered  respond  more 
slowly  to  events  in  the  visual  field  of  their  compound 
eyes,  led  to  the  notion  that  ocelli  serve  to  measure 
general  intensity  of  illumination  and  to  control  the 
level  of  tonic  contraction  in  locomotor  muscles. 

Variation  in  proportion  of  parts  and  arrangement 
of  retinal  cells  seems  to  have  little  effect  in  determin- 
ing the  role  of  insect  ocelli  (167-170,  303,  304). 
Some  ocelli  show  a  strong  retinal  astigmatism  (fig.  5, 
lower  right'),  those  in  some  dragonflies  (order  Odonata) 
being  particularly  pronounced  (252).  In  the  orthop- 
teran  Acridium  the  ocelli  are  dimorphic  in  that  those 
of  the  female  alone  show  a  double  curvature  on  the 
proximal  surface  of  the  corneal  lens — like  a  bifocal 
spectacle  lens — producing  two  images  at  different 
distances  (253).  No  explanation  is  available. 

Ocelli  in  which  the  components  of  three-part 
lenses  lie  side  by  side,  like  the  top  of  a  clover-leaf 
roll,  are  found  in  the  larvae  of  many  urochordates. 
Mast  (181)  reported  photosensory  responses  of  this 
type  of  larva  until  the  time  of  metamorphosis  when 
the  ocelli  degenerate.  Whether  the  remarkable  lenses 
indicate  fusion  from  an  originally   triplicate  ocellar 


632 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


cluster  is  not  clear  from  embryological  studies  (78). 
A  comparable  suggestion  of  multiple  origin  for  the 
one  to  three  ocelli  in  pelagic  salps  has  received  no 
support. 

COMPOUND  OCELLI  OR  AGGREGATE  EYES.  The  grouping 

of  separate  ocelli,  each  with  its  own  retina  and  pig- 
ment cup,  into  a  pattern  with  roughly  radial  diver- 
gence of  the  optic  axes  seems  to  have  arisen  inde- 
pendently in  many  phyla  through  convergent 
evolution.  In  coelenterates  a  number  of  scyphozoans 
(cubomedusae)  exhibit  this  arrangement  (132).  It  is 
characteristic  of  chaetognaths  in  which  three  ocelli 
are  clustered  in  each  of  two  groups  (120,  124). 
Among  arthropods,  compound  ocelli  resemble  a 
compound  eye  in  many  millipedes  and  in  males  of  the 
insect  order  Strepsiptera  (227,  246).  The  millipede 
Narceus  is  hatched  with  only  a  single  ocellus  but  adds 
others  in  a  triangular  area  on  each  side  of  the  head 
until  a  total  of  between  40  and  50  are  present  a  few 
instars  before  sexual  maturity  (32). 

Many  starfish  (asteroid  echinoderms)  bear  a  cluster 
of  ocelli  at  the  tip  of  their  arms.  Muscular  movements 
of  the  arm  tissues  can  alter  somewhat  the  relative 
orientation  of  the  separate  ocelli  (296),  Ijut  the  form 
of  each  photosensory  unit  appears  fixed.  A  large 
central  dioptric  body  must  serve  to  concentrate  radia- 
tions upon  the  receptor  cells  (242,  258),  and  photo- 
sensory  functions  seems  indicated  by  the  slow  action 
potentials  which  develop  upon  illumination  of  the 
ocellar  area  (90).  Some  movements  related  to  the 
direction  of  lateral  illumination  and  of  shadows  have 
been  described,  but  at  least  some  of  these  may  be 
due  to  a  general  photosensitivity  in  the  dermis.  The 
presence  of  carotenoid  pigments  in  the  compound 
ocellar  tissues  (192)  could  relate  to  a  photosensitive 
substance.  Alternatively  these  pigments  may  serve  as 
filters  which  affect  the  spectral  sensitivity  of  the 
organism. 

STEMMATA.  In  the  larvae  of  many  members  of  the 
holometabolous  insect  orders,  Neuroptera  (j.  lat^, 
Coleoptera,  Lepidoptera,  Trichoptera,  Diptera  and 
Hymenoptera,  photosensory  structures  resembling 
isolated  ommatidia  are  present.  They  disappear  at 
metamorphosis  and  have  no  relationship  to  the 
compound  eyes  of  the  adult  stage.  Their  distinctness 
from  an  ontogenetic  standpoint  led  Landois  (160)  to 
consider  them  as  an  independent  type  of  eye;  he 
called  them  'composite  eyes,'  but  the  term  stemmata 
has  been  approved  more  widely. 

Anatomical  details  have  been  described  for  those 


of  a  larval  water  beetle  Acilius  (236),  a  lepidopteran 
caterpillar  Isia  (54,  55),  a  mosquito  wriggler  Cukx 
(35)  and  several  sawfly  larvae  (Hymenoptera)  in  a 
comparative  study  by  Cornell  (36).  For  the  cater- 
pillar, Dethier  considered  the  diopteric  system  and 
found  that  a  stemma  with  a  one-part  lens  had  an 
effective  aperture  between  f  0.5  and  f/i.o,  whereas 
those  with  a  three-part  lens  were  slightly  less  spec- 
tacular collectors  of  li»ht  with  effective  apertures  be- 
tween i.o  and  1.5.  In  all  instances  the  caterpillar 
stemma  had  seven  receptor  cells  arranged  at  two 
levels,  a  distal  clump  of  three  and  a  proximal  group 
of  four.  No  matter  whether  the  corneal  lens  was 
simple  or  tripartite,  only  a  single  crystalline  body  was 
below  it,  close  to  the  distal  group  of  receptor  cells. 
The  stemmata  were  fi.xed  in  the  firm  head  capsule 
at  such  angles  to  each  other  that  their  fields  did  not 
overlap.  Dethier  concluded  that  a  coarse  type  of 
mosaic  vision  was  possible. 

Many  caterpillars  show  clear  responses  to  distant 
trees.  Those  of  the  nun  moth  Lymantria  under  ex- 
perimental conditions  will  react  to  and  approach 
pillars  and  vertical  stripes  of  paint  (131),  whereas 
horizontal  patterns  seem  to  be  ignored.  Hundert- 
mark,  who  explored  this  problem  thoroughly,  con- 
cluded that  dark  vertical  patterns  stimulated  the 
larvae  while  their  heads  were  being  swung  from  side 
to  side — a  characteristic  gesture  of  these  caterpillars. 
Stimulation  would  then  correspond  to  patterns  cross- 
ing the  visual  field  of  stemma  after  stemma,  and  the 
astigmatism  noted  would  have  a  basis  in  behavior 
rather  than  in  structure. 

COMPOUND  EYES.  True  compound  eyes  are  restricted 
to  arthropods  (fig.  6,  lower  right)  and  are  represented 
among  crustaceans,  trilobites,  xiphosurans,  euryp- 
terids,  inany  fossil  chilopods  and  diplopods,  the 
centipede  Scutigera  and  close  allies,  and  most  insects. 
Holometabolous  insects  possess  them  only  as  adults. 
In  all  situations  they  present  a  much  more  eflfective 
organization  than  compoimd  eyespots  or  compound 
ocelli  Ijut  show  the  same  quasi-radial  dixeraience  of 
visual  units. 

According  to  Hanstrom  (82),  all  true  compound 
eyes  arise  from  a  lateral  ectodermal  mass  in  the 
embryo.  In  following  these  embryonic  steps  toward 
the  final  battery  of  ommatidia,  Watase  (278)  recog- 
nized no  major  variants  in  development.  One  rather 
fundamental  difference  has  been  overlooked  in  these 
and  subsequent  studies:  in  Limulus  (and  presumably 
all  xiphosurans,  perhaps  eurypterids  as  well),  the 
entire  dioptric  mechanism  is  molted.  Other  arthro- 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES  633 


PELECYPOD      (ARCA) 


ANNELID    (BRA  NCHIOMMA  ) 


PIGMENT    CELL- 
SENSORY  CELL- 


MANTLE 
EPITHELIUM 


OPTIC    NERVE 


CRUSTACEAN    ( ASTACUS) 


GANGLIA 


CUTICLE 


OPTIC  NERVE 
FIBER 


SENSORY 
/CELL 


OPTIC  NERVE 
FIBER 


FIG.  6.  Quasiradial  divergence  of  photosensory  units  is  characteristic  of  both  compound  eyespots 
and  compound  eyes.  The  former  are  exemplified  by  the  pelecypod  moUusk  Area  (upper  right^  and 
the  polychaete  annelid  Branchiomma  QeJC),  shown  in  lengthwise  and  transverse  section  and  in  detail. 
Each  ommatidium  of  the  compound  eye  Qower  right,  detaiO  consists  of  a  cuticular  lens,  additional 
dioptric  components  on  the  ommatidial  axis,  a  cluster  of  receptor  cells  whose  nerve  fibers  penetrate 
the  basal  membrane  and  an  investing  sheath  of  pigment  cells.  .At  the  extreme  right  are  sections  cut 
through  such  an  ommatidium  at  lesels  as  indicated.  Commonly  the  optic  nerve  fibers  pass  to  a 
series  of  ganglia  close  to  the  eye  Qower  right,  longitudinal  section  of  crustacean  eye  and  stalk).  [Braji- 
chiornn  after  Hesse,  Area  after  Kijpfer,  Astaciis  after  Giesbrecht;  from  Milne  &  Milne  (193).] 


pods  which  molt  after  acquiring  compound  eyes  re- 
tain most  of  the  dioptric  mechanism,  shedding  only 
the  corneal  lens  or  a  part  of  it. 

There  is  reason  to  question  that  the  con\entional 
classification  of  arthropod  ominatidia  has  a  sound 
phylogenetic  basis.  The  term  'exoconc'  is  applied  to 
those  of  crustaceans,  trilobites,  and  beetles  of  the 
families  Dermestidae,  Elateridae  and  Lampyridae,  in 
which  the  dioptric  parts  consist  of  a  molted  corneal 
lens  and  a  nonmolted  inward  extension  of  corneal 
secretion.  Elsewhere  special  cone  cells  ('Semper's 
cells')  lie  between  the  corneal  lens  and  the  receptor 
cells,  and  provide  dioptric  function.  In  'acone'  om- 
matidia  the  cone  cells  become  transparent  and  refract 


light;  they  occupy  all  of  the  space  and  are  character- 
istic of  the  insect  orders  Dermaptera,  Heteroptera, 
some  Odonata,  some  Coleoptera  and  .some  nemato- 
cerous  Diptera.  In  'eucone'  ommatidia  the  cone  cells 
secrete  a  solid  'crystalline  cone'  within  themselves, 
usually  in  such  a  wa\'  that  the  cone-cell  nuclei  remain 
distal  to  the  cone;  .sometimes  an  anuclear  portion  of 
the  cone  cells  lies  proximal  of  the  cone;  this  type  is 
characteristic  of  the  insect  orders  Thysanura,  CoUem- 
bola,  Orthoptera,  Homoptera,  Neuroptera,  Trichop- 
tera,  Lepidoptera,  Hymenoptera,  some  members  of 
Odonata,  inost  of  the  Coleoptera  and  some  neinato- 
cerous  Diptera.  The  brachycerous  Diptera  are  unique 
in  ha\ing  'pseudocone'  ommatidia  in  which  the  cone 


634 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


cells  remain  small  and  transparent  but  secrete  distal 
to  themselves  a  fluid  or  paste  extending  to  the  corneal 
lens  and  supposedly  aiding  in  the  refraction  of  light. 

At  the  proximal  end  of  the  dioptric  mechanism 
within  the  ommatidium  is  a  ring  of  receptor  cells, 
or  two  rings,  one  distal  to  the  other.  Two  rings  may 
be  more  primiti\'e  than  one  ring.  In  Limulus  and  some 
others,  an  eccentric  receptor  cell  lies  outside  the  ring 
but  extends  a  terminal  segment  toward  the  dioptric 
system  in  a  position  central  to  the  ring  of  other  recep- 
tor cells.  More  commonly  there  is  no  eccentric  cell, 
and  the  ring  of  receptors  secretes  a  translucent  rod 
in  the  core  position  as  a  rhabdom  which  conducts 
light  energy  along  the  optical  axis  to  the  more  proxi- 
mal parts  of  the  receptor  cells. 

In  rendering  these  dioptric  structures  visiljle  in 
sections  through  a  compound  eye,  it  is  customary  to 
bleach  the  pigment  from  the  cells  which  sheath  each 
ommatidium.  Exner  (68)  appears  to  have  forgotten 
the  existence  of  this  pigment  mantle  while  tracing  the 
path  of  light  rays,  van  der  Horst  (256)  drew  attention 
to  the  pronounced  diaphragmatic  effect  of  the  mantle 
in  many  compound  eyes,  limiting  the  passage  of  light 
to  so  small  an  aperture  that  no  image  could  be  pro- 
duced at  the  receptor  level.  Under  these  circum- 
stances an  indi\'idual  ommatidium  could  do  no  better 
than  serve  as  a  photometer.  Compound  eyes  which 
are  used  in  daylight  operate  in  this  way,  with  each 
ommatidium  isolated  from  its  neighbors,  and  any 
picture  of  the  outside  world  a  synthesized  one  in  the 
central  nervous  system,  built  on  the  mosaic  of  photo- 
metric information  coming  from  the  indi\idual  om- 
matidia. 

Notthaft  (202)  took  the  extreme  view  that  each 
ommatidium  operated  on  an  all-or-none  principle. 
Either  a  target  was  included  in  its  visual  field  enough 
to  stimulate  the  receptor  system,  or  not.  Almost 
certainly  this  view  is  too  severe.  In  Limulus,  where  the 
compound  eye  may  be  somewhat  degenerate,  the 
optic  nerve  fibers  lack  lateral  connections  and  gan- 
glion cells  for  a  distance  from  the  eye  sufficient  that 
electrodes  can  be  applied  and  the  response  of  indi- 
vidual ommatidia  studied  (75,  89,  90,  286).  In 
juveniles,  two  or  more  nerve  fibers  per  ommatidium 
may  carry  nerve  impulses  when  the  eye  is  illuminated, 
but  in  adults  only  one  is  conducting,  seemingly  the 
one  arising  from  the  eccentric  cell.  A  wide  range  of 
sensitivity  and  of  response  is  evident.  But  the  function 
of  the  9  to  1 9  other  receptor  cells  in  each  ommatidium 
remains  unknown.  Both  at  threshold  and  under  in- 
tense illumination,  the  ommatidium  discharges  im- 
pulses as  a  unit. 


The  directional  sensitivity  of  single  ommatidia  in 
the  compound  eye  of  Limulus  has  been  evaluated  using 
the  same  electrical  technique  (282).  Sensitivity  is 
highest  on  the  optic  axis  and  falls  off  to  a  tenth  or  less 
for  light  sources  10  to  20  degrees  on  any  side.  The 
effectiv-e  aperture  of  the  ommatidium  from  a  physio- 
logical point  of  view  is  thus  to  40  degrees  for  high 
sensitivity  and  to  180  degrees  for  response  to  stimuli 
as  much  as  four  log  units  above  threshold  intensity. 
Yet  the  maximum  angular  separation  of  Limulus 
ommatidia  is  about  15  degrees,  the  minimum  4  to  5 
degrees.  Hence  the  overlap  of  visual  fields  of  neigh- 
boring units  must  be  extensive  and  the  acuity  which 
might  be  predicted  (as  Notthaft  did)  on  the  basis  of 
number  of  ommatidia  is  probably  not  realized.  Since 
the  dioptric  mechanism  of  the  Limulus  ommatidium 
is  somewhat  different  from  that  of  most  other  arthro- 
pods, however,  these  findings  may  not  apply  widely. 
Acuity  may  be  far  better  elsewhere  in  the  phylum. 

The  compound  eye  seems  particularly  efficient  in 
detecting  movements  in  its  total  visual  field.  This 
can  be  demonstrated  under  field  conditions  (34)  or  as 
a  sensitivity  to  flickered  light  in  the  laboratory  (298- 
301).  When  plotted  on  a  probability  grid,  flicker- 
fusion  curves  are  like  visual-acuity  curves  in  being 
essentially  straight  lines  (298,  299).  This  may  be  due 
to  a  normal  statistical  distribution  of  sensitivities 
among  the  ommatidia;  or  it  may  arise  through  the 
recruitment  of  progressively  more  ommatidia  in  a 
convex  eye  as  the  intensity  of  stimulus  rises.  Crozier 
&  W'olf  (42)  believed  that  the  latter  was  the  limiting 
factor  in  the  crayfish  Camharus. 

The  intensity  difference  required  for  flicker  detec- 
tion by  arthropods  is  greater  than  that  for  the  human 
eye.  At  optimum  intensity  the  honeybee  requires  one 
stimulus  to  be  25  per  cent  greater  or  less  than  the 
other  (298,  299).  For  the  fly  Drosophila  the  difference 
must  be  of  the  order  of  225  per  cent  (98,  99).  For 
man  1.5  per  cent  is  adequate  in  good  illumination. 
Hence  the  visual  field  of  the  arthropod  eye  contains 
a  gray  scale  with  far  fewer  than  the  500  steplike 
increments  between  black  and  white  detectable  by 
the  human  eye. 

Evaluation  of  stimuli  effective  with  a  compound 
eye  is  more  satisfactory  if  it  can  be  made  from  elec- 
troretinograms  rather  than  kinetic  responses  of  the 
entire  animal.  Electrical  records  of  this  kind  are  pos- 
sible either  with  a  surviving  eye  (56)  or  an  intact 
animal  (87).  Antrum  &  Stocker  (7)  learned  with  this 
technique  that  insects  show  two  \ery  different  ranges 
in  flicker  detection.  The  fly  Calliphora,  the  wasp  Vespa 
and  the  honeybee  Apis  responded  to  rates  as  high  as 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


635 


200  per  sec,  better  than  five  times  the  performance 
of  the  human  eye.  In  the  cockroach  Periplaneta  and 
the  grasshopper  Tachycines,  by  contrast,  any  flickering 
rate  higher  than  5  or  10  per  sec.  was  e\idently  fused 
and  interpretated  as  a  constant  stimulus.  The  authors 
postulated  that  in  the  orthopterans  an  after-image 
was  present,  a  phenomenon  lacking  in  the  dipterans 
and  hymenopterans. 

For  the  fly  Calliphora  the  electroretinograms  show 
that  the  effective  angle  of  view  of  each  ommatidium 
is  about  twice  as  great  in  the  horizontal  plane  as  in 
the  vertical  (6).  Hence  a  target  remains  for  a  longer 
time  within  the  visual  field  if  it  is  moving  horizontally; 
summation  can  permit  its  detection  at  a  lower  thresh- 
old than  would  be  found  in  the  same  target  moving 
vertically.  The  structural  basis  for  astigmatism  of  this 
kind  can  be  found  in  the  dimensions  and  divergence 
of  ommatidia.  Ommatidia  facing  downward  com- 
monly are  relatively  shorter  and  have  larger  lenses 
than  those  facing  upward;  usually  they  diverge  from 
one  another  more  strongly.  Antrum  (6)  generalized 
that  in  all  insects  which  fly  well  the  angle  of  view  of 
each  ommatidium  in  the  horizontal  direction  is 
about  twice  that  in  the  vertical. 

In  Apis  the  situation  is  somewhat  more  complex 
(i  i).  The  radius  of  curvature  of  the  bee  eye  is  smaller 
in  the  transverse  plane  than  in  the  frontal,  with  a 
ratio  near  2.5  to  i.  The  angle  between  ommatidia  is 
regularly  greater  in  the  transverse  than  in  the  frontal 
plane,  with  a  ratio  of  difference  reaching  about  2  to  i . 
In  consequence  maximum  acuity  lies  in  a  plane  in- 
clined 65  degrees  to  the  sagittal,  and  in  this  plane 
only  in  an  arc  from  47  degrees  behind  the  anterior 
margin  of  the  eye  to  49  degrees  ahead  of  the  pos- 
terior margin. 

The  extremes  of  difference  in  dimensions  and 
angular  separation  among  ommatidia  in  a  single 
compound  eye  are  met  in  some  deep-sea  crustaceans 
and  in  insects  belonging  to  the  orders  Homoptera 
(287),  Ephemeroptera  and  Diptera  (57,  58).  In  most 
of  these  the  region  with  short  ommatidia  and  large 
lenses  is  confined  to  one  part  of  the  eye,  and  the  por- 
tion with  long  ommatidia,  slight  divergences  and 
fine  lenses  forms  a  sort  of  'turban'  toward  the  top  of 
the  head.  In  many  instances  the  owner  of  such  a 
'divided'  eye  is  a  predator.  However,  Radl  (221) 
concluded  that  it  indicated  a  duality  of  embryonic 
origin  and  proposed  a  "duplicity  theory.'  Zavfel 
(308)  extended  this  into  a  triplicity  theory,  but  later 
workers  have  not  supported  either  hypothesis. 

de  Serres  (53)  appears  to  have  been  the  earliest  to 
experiment  with  arthropod  vision  by  painting  over 


all  or  part  of  a  compound  eye  with  black  varnish.  He 
found  many  of  the  postural  changes  which  became 
classic  demonstrations  of  the  'muscle  tonus'  theory  in 
rather  recent  texts  of  physiology.  Light  intensity, 
interpreted  through  the  compound  eyes,  w-as  be- 
lieved to  control  the  tonus  of  muscles  involved  in 
posture  and  locomotion.  'Circus'  movements  of  uni- 
laterally blinded  arthropods  were  explained  on  this 
basis. 

At  the  same  time  de  Serres  pointed  out  the  'false 
pupil'  seen  as  a  shifting  dot  or  line  in  many  living 
compound  eyes.  Ewing  (66)  described  it  more  fully 
and  concluded  correctly  that  it  represents  ommatidial 
pigment  visible  in  those  few  ommatidia  facing  an 
observer's  eye.  In  cylindrical  compound  eyes,  such  as 
the  stalked  ones  of  the  crab  Ocypoda,  it  takes  the  form 
of  a  vertical  line  which  follows  the  observer  or  camera 
lens  through  as  much  as  a  360-degree  field  of  view. 

Duges  (60)  noted  that  a  false  pupil  can  be  seen 
simultaneously  in  the  two  compound  eyes  of  many 
insects  and  suggested  that  they  must  have  binocular 
vision.  Demoll  (47),  working  from  sections  of  com- 
pound eyes,  showed  the  extent  of  these  binocular 
fields.  It  is  easy  to  assume  that  binocular  vision  is 
important  to  predaceous  arthropods  and  that  they 
snatch  for  prey  when  the  proper  ommatidia  in  the 
two  eyes  are  stimulated  simultaneously  by  an  object 
placed  symmetrically  in  the  binocular  field.  Distance 
estimation  is  evidently  good  in  both  naiad  and  adult 
stages  of  most  members  of  the  insect  order  Odonata 
(i,  8,  50),  among  the  dipteran  family  Asilidae  (185) 
and  in  tiger  beetles. 

The  adaptaiaility  of  the  neural  components  in  the 
eye-brain  team  appears  to  have  been  underestimated. 
In  a  matter  of  hours  or  days,  the  postural  peculiarities 
and  circus  movements  of  unilaterally  blinded  arthro- 
pods often  disappear  entirely.  Partially  blinded  odona- 
tan  naiads  can  adapt  their  behavior  to  approach 
prey  monocularly,  pivot  and  seize  at  the  appropriate 
instant  (i,  8).  Whether  marginal  ommatidia  can 
participate  in  this  versatility  has  been  questioned  (9). 
It  would  be  interesting  to  know  whether  variations  in 
adaptability  correspond  to  the  zones  found  in  the 
Notonecta  eye  (172),  since  in  this  heteropteran  insect 
kinetic  responses  seem  related  to  specific  areas  of  the 
compound  eye.  Certainly  ommatidia  can  serve  in  un- 
usual reflexes  (2191  220)  but  limitations  may  still  be 
present. 

Many,  perhaps  most,  arthropod  eyes  show  a  sensi- 
tivity to  the  plane  of  polarization  of  light  from  the 
sky.  Something  of  the  kind  has  been  suspected  for 
many  years  to  account  for  the  homing  ability  of  vari- 


636 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


ous  hynienopteran  insects.  Wolsky  C302)  sought  in 
vain  to  find  an  analyzer  function  in  tiie  corneal  lenses 
of  the  coarse  compound  eyes  of  land  isopod  crusta- 
ceans, von  Frisch  credited  the  actual  demonstration 
of  polarization  sensitivity  to  Autruni,  although  von 
Frisch's  own  reports  (271,  272)  antedate  any  of 
Autrum's  published  comments  on  the  subject,  von 
Frisch  has  found  in  the  honeybee's  '.sky  compass'  a 
basis  for  the  ability  of  one  bee  to  communicate  to 
another  by  dancing  within  the  dark  hi\e  the  direction 
to  a  discovered  supply  of  food  (273). 

Since  von  Frisch  used  an  octagon  of  Polaroid  film 
cut  into  svmmetrically  fitted  equilateral  triangles,  it 
was  natural  that  he  should  parallel  this  with  the  ring 
of  receptor  cells  in  each  insect  ommatidium.  When 
the  octagon  was  held  toward  the  blue  .sky,  some  one 
diagonal  was  darkest,  another  lighest.  The  former 
corresponded  to  Polaroid  triangles  the  axes  of  which 
were  transverse  to  the  plane  of  polarization  in  the 
sky  area  seen  through  the  plastic.  Possibly  an  image 
of  comparable  type  was  cast  upon  the  ring  of  receptor 
cells  and  information  of  this  kind  interpreted  ijy  a 
single  ommatidium. 

In  the  dipteran  VohueUa,  a  ring  of  eight  receptor 
cells  and  birefringence  with  extinction  of  one  ray 
were  reported  (186),  but  the  reality  of  the  phe- 
nomenon described  is  open  to  question.  Certainly 
each  ommatidium  may  vary  in  its  response  to  light 
as  the  plane  of  polarization  of  incident  radiation  is 
rotated  around  its  optic  axis,  as  may  occur  in  Limulus 
(279,  280,  282,  283)  or  in  Drosoplula  (244).  But  struc- 
tural asymmetries  of  dioptric  components  in  indi- 
vidual ommatidia  and  the  oL)liquit\  with  which 
many  ommatidia  in  each  eye  meet  the  outer  surface 
seem  more  responsible  for  the  sensitivity  found  to  the 
plane  of  polarization.  The  extent  to  which  polarized 
light  is  available  as  a  cue  useful  in  arthropod  naviga- 
tion both  in  air  and  in  water  has  been  described  by 
Waterman  (280,  285).  It  has  been  shown  experimen- 
tally (12)  to  be  significant  in  the  free  beha\ior  of 
fresh-water  planktonic  crustaceans. 

In  the  earliest  comprehensive  account  of  the  arthro- 
pod compound  eye,  Grenacher  (81)  recognized  a 
difference  in  the  distribution  of  pigment  cells  accord- 
ing to  whether  the  organism  was  a  day-active  type 
or  a  crepuscular  and  night-active  organism.  In  most 
of  the  latter,  the  pigment  is  not  extended  as  a  sheath 
isolating  each  ommatidium  from  the  next  but  is 
clumped  in  such  a  way  that  light  could  pass  obliquely 
from  ommatidium  to  ommatidium.  Exner  (67,  68) 
traced  the  ray  paths,  and  showed  by  diagrams  how 
light  entering  many  ommatidial  lenses  could  be  re- 


fracted and  fall  on  the  receptors  of  a  central  visual 
unit.  Grenacher's  terms  apposition'  type  for  the  eye 
with  isolated  ommatidia  and  'superposition'  type  for 
the  eyes  used  in  dim  illumination  have  been  retained. 

The  same  ommatidium  may  function  alone  by  day 
and  in  concert  by  night  through  migratory  move- 
ments of  its  pigment  (145,  248).  In  crustaceans  these 
changes  in  the  eye  are  often  matched  by  alterations 
in  body  color,  the  entire  chromatophore  system  being 
imdcr  the  control  of  hormones  whose  secretion  is  in- 
fluenced by  stimulation  of  the  eyes  by  light  (207). 
The  literature  on  this  subject  has  become  extensive 
l)ut  most  of  it  centers  on  hormonal  aspects.  In  insects 
the  corresponding  shifts  in  ommatidial  pigment  may 
be  independent  of  hormones  (44,  49). 

Normal  structure  of  compound  eyes  has  required 
extensive  study  because  of  the  large  numJDer  of  varia- 
tions within  the  wealth  of  genera  in  the  phylum 
Arthropoda.  Where  possible,  many  writers  on  the 
subject  have  attempted  to  correlate  form  with  func- 
tion (30,  31,  48,  51,  61,  62,  119,  250,  268,  269). 
Numerous  crustaceans  bear  their  eyes  on  movable 
eyestalks  and  show  compensatory  movements  of  these 
when  the  animal  or  its  visual  field  is  rotated.  Branchio- 
pods  show  all  gradations  between  a  distinct  pair  of 
compound  eyes  and  indistinguishable  fusion  into  a 
single  mass.  The  fused  median  compound  eye  of 
Daphnia  consists  of  al)Out  20  ommatidia  and  is  some- 
what unusual  in  that  it  can  be  rotated  several  degrees 
within  the  body  through  the  action  of  a  series  of 
oculomotor  muscles. 

Ostracod  compound  eyes  are  commonly  separate 
if  a  median  ocellus  is  present  but  fused  if  the  ocellus 
is  lacking.  Some  lack  compoimd  eyes  entirely.  The 
luminescent  Cypndina,  howe\er,  has  full)-  developed 
eyes. 

Copepod  compound  eyes  range  from  the  median 
fused  structure  of  Cyclops  and  Calamis  through  genera 
in  which  the  two  are  completely  separate.  Branchiuran 
compound  eyes  must  be  regarded  as  degenerate. 
Argulus  has  four  eye  types  present  in  each  individual. 
Barnacles  ha\e  compound  eyes  onl\  during  the  meta- 
nauplian  stage  (69).  .'\mong  chilopods  only  Scuttgera 
and  related  genera  possess  compound  eyes  (84).  Here 
each  eye  consists  of  not  more  than  200  ommatidia, 
each  with  two  rings  of  receptor  cells  as  in  the  thysanu- 
ran  insect  Lepisina. 

Growth  of  compound  eyes  is  inferred  among  trilo- 
bites  because  of  the  gradual  increase  in  number  ot 
ommatidia  found  to  accompany  increase  in  body 
size  within  each  species  (226).  In  Limulus  and  other 
xiphosurans,  ijoth  the  number  of  ommatidia  and  the 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


637 


size  of  each  increase  at  each  moh  (284),  rapidly  in 
early  ages  and  more  slowly  later  on.  The  same  is 
true  in  crustaceans  studied  (14,  16,  149)  and  most 
insects  (16,  173).  The  stick  insect  Dixippiis  is  unusual 
in  adding  no  new  ommatidia,  although  the  total  in- 
crease in  dimensions  of  each  is  126  per  cent  and  the 
eye  area  doubles  from  hatching  to  maturity. 

Development  of  the  compound  eye  appears  to  de- 
pend upon  normality  of  the  supraesophageal  gan- 
glion. Damage  to  this  ganglion  usually  leads  to  failure 
of  the  eye  to  differentiate.  In  Drosophila,  however,  the 
various  genetic  mutants  with  degenerate  eyes  arise 
through  factors  acting  on  the  eye  itself  and  not  in- 
directly through  the  nervous  system  (225).  Degenera- 
tion of  compound  eyes  in  cavernicolous  arthropods 
and  deep-sea  crustaceans  is  common  and  apparently 
follows  a  similar  genetic  course  influencing  the  eye 
itself  (83).  Beddard  (13)  believed  a  relationship  could 
be  seen  between  depth  and  degree  of  degeneration  of 
the  compound  eye,  but  so  many  instances  of  hyper- 
trophy of  these  organs  in  deep-sea  crustaceans  have 
been  described  that  the  generalization  is  unsafe. 

Regeneration  following  injury  to  the  compound 
eyes  seems  possible  in  decapod  crustaceans,  although 
the  regenerated  part  is  not  an  eye  but  an  antennalike 
organ.  Trilobites  alone  are  known  to  have  regenerated 
ommatidia  (138),  this  being  recognized  in  terms  of 
independence  in  the  direction  of  the  facet  pattern  in 
areas  set  off  by  scar  tissue. 

CAMERA-STYLE  EYES  IN  MOLLUSKS.  The  remarkable 
convergence  in  anatomical  organization  between  the 
large  eyes  of  some  cephalopod  moUusks  and  those  of 
vertebrate  animals  have  led  to  frequent  comment. 
Hensen  (102)  investigated  the  embryonic  steps  lead- 
ing to  the  cephalopod  types  of  eye.  In  all  the  organ 
arises  as  an  invaginated  vesicle.  That  of  Nautilus  is 
unique  in  proceeding  no  farther  and  hence  remaining 
as  a  pinhole-camera  eye  (fig.  7,  righl^. 

In  all  other  cephalopods  the  vesicle  closes  and 
sinks  below  the  body  surface.  The  douijle  layer  of 
tissue  where  the  pinhole  closed  produces  a  pair  of 
planoconvex  lenses  in  contact  with  one  another,  as 
the  sole  structure  focusing  an  image  in  these  marine 
organisms.  Distal  to  the  lens  an  encircling  ridge  arises 
forming  the  muscular  iris  diaphragm  (fig.  7,  center^. 
The  whole  eye  sinks  further  below  the  surface  at  the 
bottom  of  a  fresh  invagination  the  rim  of  which 
closes  over  either  partialh'  or  completely  in  forming 
a  transparent  cornea.  A  number  of  genera  retain  an 
open  pore  between  the  anterior  chamber  and  the 
outside  world,  and  sea  water  washes  the  front  of  the 


lens.  In  some  genera  an  additional  encircling  ridge 
forms  around  the  eye,  producing  an  approach  to  eye- 
lids. 

Deep-sea  cephalopods  often  have  eyes  which  are 
amazingly  hypertrophied,  sometimes  supported  on 
swi\eling  turrets  (33).  In  these  a  binocular  field  seems 
probable,  whereas  in  most  surface  and  mid-water 
cephalopods  the  visual  fields  are  completely  .separate. 
The  apparent  absence  of  blind  cephalopods  must  be 
related  to  the  number  of  kinds  which  bear  lumines- 
cent organs  in  the  depths. 

Most  cephalopods  have  a  slit  pupil  which  closes 
into  a  slightly  hooked  horizontal  line.  It  is  under 
direct  control  from  the  central  nervous  system  and 
changes  the  degree  of  opening  more  in  relation  to 
emotional  conditions  than  it  does  refle.xly  in  relation 
to  light  intensity  (259).  Muscles  provide  for  accom- 
modation of  the  lens  (151,  164)  and  demonstrate  their 
action  when  the  outer  surface  of  the  eye  is  stimulated 
electrically  (2,  101).  In  Octopus,  at  least,  the  resting 
eye  is  myopic  by  6  to  10  diopters,  and  accommoda- 
tion is  both  positive  for  objects  at  clo.se  range  and 
negative  for  distance  (274). 

Unlike  the  vertebrate  eye,  the  cephalopod  organ 
has  a  direct  retina.  Its  optic  nerve  fibers  may  emerge 
from  the  eyeball  as  multiple  bundles  which  fuse  into 
a  common  optic  nerve.  Around  them  are  the  four 
oculomotor  muscles  which  shift  the  eye  in  a  wide 
range  of  movements,  including  rotational  ones  (121, 

25O. 

Electroretinograms  from  cephalopod  eyes  (211, 
212)  has'e  been  as  helpful  as  beha\ior  in  indicating 
the  role  of  vision  in  these  animals.  In  all  cephalopods 
the  nervous  system  is  .so  highly  organized,  with  \  isual 
cues  related  elaborately  to  tactile  ones  and  perhaps 
taste  as  well,  that  simple  responses  are  rarely  elicited. 
Captise  animals  are  seemingly  aff"ected  strongly  by 
their  confinement,  but  will  develop  conditioned  re- 
sponses under  skilful  handling. 

Camera-style  eyes  of  quite  different  form  are  found 
in  some  other  mollusks.  a)  In  the  sand-eating  pul- 
monate  gastropods  Onchidium,  Oncis  and  Peronina,  the 
dorsal  surface  of  the  body  bears  short  wartlike  projec- 
tions each  with  a  single  eye  or  with  from  two  to  ses'cn 
of  them  in  an  irregular  cluster.  Each  eye  is  about  0.2 
mm  in  diameter  and  has  a  two-part  refractive  body  be- 
tween the  rather  flattened  cornea  and  the  inverted 
retina.  The  more  distal  refractive  body  alters  in  shape 
when  a  muscular  collar  surrounding  it  contracts. 
Presumably  this  is  an  accommodation  mechanism. 
Natural  history  observations  on  a  Bermudan  On- 
chidium  posses.sing  eyes  of  this  type  suggest  nothing 


638 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


PECTEN 


LOLIGO 


NA  U  TIL  US 


FIG.  7.  Mollusk  eyes  present  a  variety  of  optical  systems.  In  the  scallop  Pectai  Qejt),  a  cellular 
lens  concentrates  light  on  two  levels  of  inverted  retina.  The  distal  layer  mediates  only  the  oflT-responsc 
leading  to  sudden  closure  of  the  shell  valves.  The  proximal  layer  responds  to  steady  illumination. 
A  reflecting  tapetum  (shown  as  opaque  blocks  basal  to  the  pro.ximal  retina)  increases  sensitivity  and 
contrast  discrimination  at  low  intensities  of  light.  In  the  cephalopod  NaulUus  (rig/iQ,  the  eye  becomes 
functional  and  matures  as  a  pinhole-camera  organ  at  an  embryonic  stage  passed  through  in  the 
development  of  all  other  cephalopod  eyes.  In  the  squid  Loligo  (center'),  extrinsic  muscles  orient  the 
whole  eye;  intrinsic  muscles  provide  both  positive  and  negative  accommodation  and  adjust  the 
aperture  of  the  slit  pupil.  [Peclen  after  Kupfer,  Loligo  and  Nautilus  after  Hensen;  firom  Milne  & 
Milne  (193).] 


which  might  not  be  due  to  general  photosensitivity 

(5)- 

A)  Clams  of  the  genus  Cardium  bear  small  eyes  on 
short  tentacles  around  the  rim  of  the  mantle.  Each 
eye  has  a  cellular  lens  mass  in  which  the  refractive 
index  changes  from  distal  to  pro.ximal  and  may  pro- 
vide an  image  on  the  rather  coarse  inverted  retina. 
A  cup-shaped  extension  of  pigment  cells  surrounds 
the  lens  material  and  narrows  to  a  distal  pupil.  The 
whole  eye  is  invested  in  a  muscular  coat,  the  contrac- 
tion of  which  alters  the  shape  of  the  lens  mass  and 
may  serve  in  accommodation.  Nothing  is  known  of 
the  function  of  these  eyes. 

c)  In  the  pelagic  heteropods  (gastropods)  Ptero- 
trachea,  Carinaria  and  Atalania,  one  small  eye  is  borne 
projecting  from  the  body  contours  on  each  side,  like 
the  port  and  starboard  running  lights  on  a  ship. 
These  highly  modified  eyes  are  directed  forward  and 
must  have  a  binocular  field  in  front  of  the  animal, 
although  no  evidence  has  been  presented  to  show 
that  the  animal  makes  use  of  them  in  sighting  on 
objects  of  importance  to  it.  Each  eye  has  a  large 
spherical  lens  at  considerable  distance  from  a  'ladder 
retina'    with   little   ridges   of   receptor   cells   (fig.    8, 


centei)  and  a  muscle  who.se  contractions  shift  the  lens 
(109),  perhaps  as  a  fine  adjustment  for  focus. 

c.\MER.'^ -STYLE  EYES  IN  ."ANNELIDS.  Fairly  conventional 
camera-style  eyes  arc  found  in  the  pelagic  polychaetes 
Alciopa  and  Eupolvodonles.  In  the  former  (fig.  8,  left) 
the  two  eyes  at  rest  diverge  widely,  but  contraction 
of  three  extrinsic  muscles  to  each  of  them  pro\ides  a 
basis  for  convergence,  binocular  vision  and  perhaps 
distance  estimation  (45).  In  Eupolyodotites  the  eyes 
face  forward  at  rest  (210).  In  all  members  of  the 
family  Alciopidae,  the  eye  structure  is  comparable 
(79,  80).  The  large  retina  has  direct  receptors,  a 
secreted  mass  of  two  consistencies  separating  the 
retina  from  the  lens,  an  accommodation  muscle  (45), 
and  a  secretory  cell  (107)  which  responds  well  to 
electrical  stimulation.  Secretory  action  increases  the 
volume  of  the  distal  mass  behind  the  lens  and  pushes 
the  lens  forward,  accommodating  the  eye  for  nearer 
vision.  Muscular  contraction  should  operate  in  the 
reverse  sense.  Unfortunately,  no  natural  history  de- 
tails are  available  to  indicate  how  and  when  these 
worms  use  their  remarkable  eves. 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


639 


GASTROPOD 
(PTEROTRACHEA  i 


CRUSTACEAN 
(COPILIA  ) 


VENTRAL  VIEW 


SECRETION    ACCOMMODATION 
MUSCLE 


-OPTIC  NERVE 


FIG.  8.  Camera-Style  eyes  are  found  in  several  phyla  of  invertebrate  animals,  but  the  mechanism 
of  accommodation  varies  considerably.  In  the  polychaete  annelid  Atciopa  it  includes  both  a  hy- 
draulic system  from  a  secretory  gland,  shifting  the  lens  distally,  and  a  muscle  operating  in  the  re- 
verse direction  (Jeft).  In  the  gastropod  mollusk  Plerolrachea,  a  muscle  pro\ides  the  basis  for  fine 
focusing  on  a  series  of  receptor  clusters  known  collectively  as  a  ladder  retina'  (center).  In  the  copepod 
crustacean  Copilia,  a  small  group  of  receptor  cells  at  the  focal  point  of  the  biconvex  lens  is  shifted 
both  toward  the  lens  and  swung  laterally  by  muscular  contraction.  In  both  the  mollusk  and  the 
crustacean  the  eyes  apparently  are  useful  only  as  sights  but,  like  the  annelid  eyes  with  their  extrinsic 
musculature,  may  have  a  binocular  field  in  advance  of  the  body.  [Alciopa  after  DcmoU;  Plerolrachea 
after  Hesse;  Copilia  dorsal  view  after  Giesbrecht,  detail  after  Grenacher;  from  Milne  &  Milne  (193).] 


CAMERA-STYLE   EYES  IN    ARTHROPODS.    Still   Icss   Can    bc 

guessed  as  to  the  function  of  strangely  simplified 
camera-style  eyes  in  the  planktonic  copepod  crusta- 
ceans known  as  corycacids.  Copilia  carries  two  of 
them  facing  forward,  widely  separated  in  the  body. 
Sapphirina  has  a  pair  close  together.  In  Corycaeus  their 
lenses  are  fused  on  the  mid-line.  Yet  in  all,  the  large 
lens  in  the  body  surface  (fig.  8,  right)  appears  to  focus 
light  on  a  little  cluster  of  three  or  four  receptor  cells 
surrounded  by  a  pigment  sheath.  A  long  slender 
muscle  lengthwise  at  the  side  of  the  eye  can  shift  the 
receptor  cluster  with  reference  to  the  lens  in  a  way 
which  may  provide  for  both  some  accominodation 
and  soiTie  sighting,  perhaps  in  binocular  vision. 
Nothing  is  known  of  the  habits  which  would  suggest 
a  use  for  a  visual  mechanism  of  this  kind. 


PHENOMENA  RELATED   TO  STIMULUS  INTENSITY 

Changes  in  the  sensitivity  and  in  the  discriminatory 
capacity  of  multicellular  eyes  are  often  based  in  part 
upon  other  features  in  addition  to  photochemical 
changes  and  such  obvious  adjustments  as  those  of  iris 
diaphragms. 

Pigment  Migration  within  the  Eye 

A  redistribution  of  pigment,  either  by  active  exten- 
sion and  contraction  of  pigment  cells  or  by  shifting  of 
pigment  granules  within  the  protoplasm  of  stationary 
cells,  follows  changes  in  intensity  of  illumination  on  a 
variety  of  eyes:  in  the  ocelli  of  the  gastropod  Planorbis 
(4,  241);  in  the  stemmata  of  the  lepidopteran  cater- 


640 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSIOLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


pillar  (235);  in  the  ommatidia  of  crustaceans  and 
insects  (15,  46,  248,  254,  289,  309);  and  in  the  retina 
of  cephalopod  camera-style  eyes  (103,  106,  223).  The 
redistribution  serves  to  reduce  the  proportion  of  in- 
tense light  reaching  the  receptor  cells  and  to  increase 
the  proportion  of  dim  light  passing  to  retinal  level. 

In  most  insects  that  are  active  by  day  the  pigment 
lies  between  the  receptor  cells  when  light  intensity  is 
high,  and  migrates  below  the  basement  membrane 
when  the  intensity  is  reduced.  In  mantid  orthopterans 
and  sphingid  lepidopterans  the  mechanism  is  more 
like  that  in  decapod  crustaceans.  During  daylight  the 
pigment  is  spread  parallel  to  the  crystalline  cones  and 
maintains  isolation  of  one  ommatidium  from  the  ne.xt 
in  typical  apposition-eye  organization;  at  night  the 
pigment  becomes  concentrated  distally,  giving  the  eye 
a  far  darker  appearance  and  permitting  it  to  function 
on  the  superposition  principle. 

Either  type  of  pigment  movement  may  expose  a 
reflecting  layer  in  the  eye.  This  may  be  either  a 
'basement  tapetum'  which  serves  to  increase  sensi- 
tivity and  contrast  at  low  light  intensities  by  reflecting 
nonabsorbed  incident  light  back  through  the  receptor 
cells,  or  an  'iris  tapetum'  which  reflects  energy  out  of 
the  eye  again  before  it  has  reached  the  receptor-cell 
level.  The  latter  is  more  developed  among  crustaceans 
(291),  although  found  in  some  insects  as  well.  If  a 
basement  tapetum  is  hidden  by  pigment  movement 
at  higher  intensities  of  light,  it  is  an  'occlusible  tape- 
turn'  analogous  to  that  found  in  some  fish.  No  regu- 
larity is  noticeable  in  either  the  chemical  nature  of 
the  reflecting  pigment  or  its  systematic  position.  In 
Limulus  the  iris  tapetum  contains  only  guanine  (146); 
the  closely-related  .xiphosuran  Tachypleus  lacks  a  tape- 
tum of  any  kind  (284};  the  crayfish  Astacus  has  an  iris 
tapetum  of  uric  acid  (147);  and  the  lobster  Homarus 
one  in  which  uric  acid  is  supplemented  by  at  least 
three  additional  substances,  none  of  which  is  guanine 
(146). 

So  far,  tapeta  have  been  recognized  either  from  eye 
histology  or  'eyeshine'  in  only  two  phyla.  Among 
moUusks  it  is  present  in  the  pelecypods  Pecten  (233) 
and  Cardium  (216).  Among  arthropods  it  is  widespread 
in  crustacean  and  insect  ommatidia,  in  the  ocelli  of 
certain  insects  (216)  and  in  the  secondary  ocelli  of 
many  spiders. 

Spectral  Sfnsitivity  and  Color  I  'isiun 

Paralleling  the  spectral  absorption  characteristics 
of  the  photosensitive  pigment  in  a  receptor  system  is 
a  spectral  sensitivity  shown  through  ner\e  impulses  or 


responses  in  effector  systems.  With  care  an  action 
spectrum  can  be  plotted  showing  the  energy  required 
in  a  light  stimulus  at  each  wavelength  in  a  series  of 
tests  to  find  the  threshold  of  response.  This  graph  is  a 
spectral  sensitivity  curve;  it  regularly  shows  one  or 
more  maxima.  The  only  exception  reported,  Hydra'f. 
response  to  light  (91),  appears  to  be  a  uniform  reac- 
tion at  all  wavelengths. 

Even  where  two  receptor  systems  are  present  in  the 
same  eye,  there  is  no  a  priori  rea.son  to  expect  them 
to  have  different  photosensitive  pigments  and  hence 
a  single  action  curve.  In  many  vertebrate  eyes  the  rod 
mechanism  and  the  cone  mechanism  are  known  to 
have  different  spectral  sensitivities,  evident  as  a 
'Purkinje  shift'  in  the  wavelength  of  maximum  sensi- 
tivity and  in  the  limits  of  the  effective  spectrum  as  the 
intensity  is  altered — reduced  until  the  cones  are  in- 
active or  raised  until  they  dominate.  A  Purkinje 
shift  has  been  detected  in  only  one  invertebrate  so  far 
(72),  the  fruit  fly  Drosopliita. 

A  dual  mechanism  in  the  eye  and  a  Purkinje  shift 
does  not  indicate  color  vision;  the  dog  has  a  Purkinje 
shift  yet  is  color  blind.  Color  \ision  depends  upon  dif- 
ferential mechanisms  in  the  brain  to  which  nerve 
impulses  go  separately  from  two  or  more  unlike  series 
of  receptors  active  in  the  same  intensity  range.  Color 
\ision  enables  an  organism  to  distinguish  between 
radiant  stimuli  on  the  basis  of  inequalities  of  energy 
content  at  diiTerent  wavelengths  rather  than  upon 
intensity  alone.  A  color-blind  organism  may  distin- 
guish between  a  series  of  grays  but  will  confuse  any 
color  with  some  one  shade  of  gray  since  only  intensity 
discrimination  is  possible.  The  xiphosuran  Limulus  has 
been  shown  to  have  the  peripheral  basis  for  color 
vision  (76)  in  that  some  ommatidia  have  greater 
sensiti\ity  toward  longer  wavelengths,  .some  toward 
shorter  \va\elengths;  apparently  this  differential  sen- 
sitivity at  the  ommatidial  le\el  is  not  used  by  the 
central  nervous  system  since  no  discrimination  be- 
tween a  spectral  hue  and  a  neutral  .source  seems  pos- 
sible except  on  an  intensity  basis. 

Scarcely  any  two  individuals,  let  alone  any  two 
species,  show  the  same  range  of  spectral  response.  The 
human  eye  is  regarded  as  sensitive  to  wavelengths 
from  the  extreme  violet  sensation  at  400  m/i  to  the 
extreme  red  at  700  niju.  Many  invertebrates  are  .sensi- 
tive to  wavelengths  designated  as  ultraviolet  (shorter 
than  400  m/u),  even  when  these  are  not  a  normal  part 
of  their  environment  (as  among  aquatic  organisms 
which  are  protected  from  radiation  of  this  type  by  the 
spectral  absorption  characteristics  of  water).  Many 
insects,  which  are  active  in  sunlight  containing;  ultra- 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


641 


violet,  are  more  sensitive  to  this  part  of  the  solar 
spectrum  than  to  the  region  visible  to  man  (17,  18, 
1 74,  1 75).  In  consequence  it  becomes  important  for 
man  to  learn  more  of  what  reflects  ultraviolet,  and 
hence  may  be  visible  to  insects  though  not  to  him 
(26,  38,  175,  176). 

Amebas  travel  as  rapidly  in  the  presence  of  radia- 
tions of  long  wavelengths  (red)  as  in  darkness  but  are 
increasingly  sensitive  as  the  wavelength  of  a  stimulus 
is  shortened  (86).  Paramecium  tends  to  swim  upwards  in 
darkness,  downward  in  light,  and  the  direction  is 
altered  most  effectively  by  shorter  wavelengths  (73). 

The  platyhelminth  Planaria  has  been  studied  ex- 
tensively in  responses  to  spectral  distribution  in  light 
stimuli.  Erhardt  (64)  was  able  to  account  for  earlier 
claims  (19,  115)  that  Planaria  had  color  vision  upon 
intensity  discrimination.  Werner  (292)  concluded 
that  much  of  the  flatworm's  response  to  ultraviolet 
arose  through  general  photosensitivity  and  not  the 
eyes;  but  Merker  &  Gilbert  (187)  found  only  non- 
directional  kinetic  responses  when  the  eyes  were 
removed,  compared  to  a  definite  orientation  with  a 
single  ocellus  intact.  They  were  able  to  plot  the  visual 
fields  of  Planaria  toward  ultnn  iolet  and  believed  that 
responses  were  to  the  wavelengths  used  (366  to  313 
m/j)  rather  than  any  secondary  fluorescence. 

Two  separate  receptor  systems  were  described  for 
the  earthworm  (255).  One,  mediating  the  shadow 
reaction,  was  most  sensitive  in  the  yellow  portion  of 
the  spectrum  and  depended  upon  receptors  distrib- 
uted uniformly  in  the  skin.  The  other,  a  more  general 
photosensitivity  related  to  rate  of  locomotion  and  the 
like,  showed  greatest  sensitivity  in  the  blue  and  was 
most  developed  toward  the  two  ends  of  the  body.  In 
the  leech  Pisricola,  pigment  migration  in  surface  chro- 
matophores  is  an  effector  demonstration  for  which  a 
spectral  action  curve  can  be  drawn  (140). 

Using  the  threshold  for  retraction  of  the  siphon  as 
a  kinetic  cue  to  photosensitivity  in  the  pelecypod  Mya, 
Hecht  (95)  obtained  a  spectral  action  curve  with 
limits  somewhat  short  of  those  for  the  human  eye. 
Its  maximum  fell  at  500  inn,  suggesting  that  the 
neuronal  photoreceptors  in  the  mantle  tissue  of  the 
clam  have  a  photosensitive  pigment  similar  to  tho.se 
extracted  from  organized  eyes. 

The  fresh-water  planktonic  crustacean  Daphnia  ap- 
pears to  have  at  least  three  photosensory  systems,  one 
with  greatest  sensitivity  in  the  ultraviolet  (257),  one 
in  the  yellow  and  the  third  in  the  blue.  Only  the 
latter  two  can  have  much  importance  under  natural 
conditions  (240).  The  response  with  maximum  sensi- 
tivity  to   yellow  is   a   positive   horizontal   swimming 


toward  the  radiant  source.  The  response  to  blue  is 
negative.  Baylor  and  Smith  at  the  University  of 
Michigan  have  used  the  yellow  and  blue  responses  in 
an  underwater  trap  which  catches  a  wide  variety  of 
plankton  organisms,  crustaceans,  acarid  arachnids  and 
insect  larvae.  Possibly  photosensory  mechanisms  of 
this  kind  are  involved  in  the  \ertical  migrations  made 
daily  by  many  types  of  plankton,  down  during  day- 
light, up  at  night. 

Although  the  arthropod  cuticle  transmits  freely  a 
wide  range  of  radiations  from  infrared  to  ultraviolet, 
only  certain  fireflies  (nocturnal  Coleoptera)  have  been 
found  to  respond  to  infrared  stimuli  (28).  By  painting 
the  eyes  of  various  butterflies  with  a  clear  red  lacquer, 
Eltringham  (63)  was  able  to  show  that  some  kinds 
flew  about  naturally — able  to  see  in  red  light — 
whereas  others  behaved  as  though  blinded. 

Sensitivity  to  ultraviolet  is  pronounced  in  most 
insects,  and  shown  by  many  lar\ae  as  well  (139,  184). 
Bertholf  (17,  18)  found  a  bimodal  curve  represented 
the  spectral  sensitivity  of  the  honeybee.  The  peak  in 
the  ultraviolet  was  far  higher  than  that  in  the  spectrum 
visible  to  man  and  explained  why  these  insects  re- 
spond more  to  cues  in  ultraviolet  components  of  sun- 
light than  to  reflectances  visible  to  man.  Lutz  (174- 
176)  examined  the  ultraviolet  world  of  the  insect  and 
was  aljle  to  produce  conditioned  responses  in  tropical 
hymcnopterans  (175)  to  patterns  in  white  paints  when 
one  white  reflected  ultraviolet  and  another  did  not. 

Conditioned  responses  in  honeybees  demonstrate 
that  these  insects  do  have  color  vision  (270,  273).  They 
can  be  trained  to  come  for  food  to  line  spectra  regard- 
less of  relative  intensity  (154-158).  But  a  majority  of 
insects,  particularly  the  night-active  ones,  probably 
show  no  color  vision,  merely  intensity  discrimination 
based  on  a  simple  spectral-.sensiti\ity  curve  (288). 
This  may  be  modified  from  one  genetic  strain  to 
another  according  to  the  eye  pigments  present  and 
changes  in  the  eye  structure  itself  (71). 

A  neural  basis  for  color  vision  has  been  described 
in  insects  (222,  230,  231);  but  whether  even  day- 
active  species,  operating  in  good  light,  make  use  of 
cues  reaching  them  from  differential  mechanisms  in 
the  ommatidia  is  a  point  which  inust  be  established 
separately  for  each  kind. 

Form  Perceplinn  and  Pattern  Recognition 

If  both  the  photosensory  mechanism  and  the  nerv- 
ous system  are  sutticiently  well  organized  and  co- 
ordinated, the  animal  can  give  evidence  of  an  aware- 
ness  of  surrounding  events   that   is  close   to,   if  not 


642 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


identical  with,  consciousness.  Identification  marks  and 
courtship  gestures  are  significant  in  these  terms.  In- 
sects which  go  habitually  from  one  flower  to  another 
of  the  same  kind  demonstrate  this  type  of  discrimina- 
tion; efficient  pollination  often  depends  upon  it. 

To  be  useful  in  form  perception  and  pattern  recog- 
nition, a  photosensory  mechanism  must  carry  inten- 
sity discrimination  the  further  step  of  detecting  simul- 
taneously in  small  areas  of  the  visible  field  the 
differences  in  intensity  which  are  significant.  Several 
types  of  acuities  are  involved,  all  of  them  properly 
defined  as  the  reciprocals  of  threshold  intensities, 
whether  linear,  areal  or  angular.  How  large  must  a 
single  object  be  to  constitute  an  adequate  stimulus? 
How  far  apart  must  two  objects  be  for  the  gap  be- 
tween them  to  be  visible?  Can  an  object,  such  as  a 
triangle,  be  significant  in  one  orientation  (say  up- 
right) but  not  another  (say  inverted}?  Is  alignment, 
or  motion  or  distance  significant  to  the  organism  as  it 
views  objects  in  the  environment? 

The  camera-style  eye  and  the  compound  eye  appear 
most  competent  to  interpret  the  world  in  terms  of 
small  differences  in  light  intensity  and  to  send  mes- 
sages to  the  central  nervous  system  from  which  a 
picture  of  the  environment  can  be  assembled.  Even 
for  organisms  with  these  eye  types,  pessimistic  views 
have  often  been  expressed.  Frequently  they  represent 
inadequacy  of  experimental  technique.  "Absence  of 
evidence  is  no  evidence  of  absence."   Thus  VVillem 


(295)  concluded  terrestrial  mollusks  could  detect  the 
presence  of  voluminous  objects  only  when  less  than  a 
centimeter  distant,  but  \on  Buddenbrock  (269)  re- 
ported compensatory  movements  of  the  eyestalks  to  a 
rotating  visual  field  much  farther  away.  And  various 
workers  (100,  258)  have  had  difficulty  satisfying 
themselves  that  cephalopod  mollusks  respond  to  visual 
cues  in  the  absence  of  simultaneous  tactile  and  gusta- 
tory stimulation. 

Plateau  (217)  obtained  so  few  responses  to  the 
stimuli  he  gave  to  captive  spiders  that  he  concluded 
that  they  were  essentially  blind.  Apparently  some  ob- 
jects are  recognized  and  others  ignored,  so  that  the 
acuities  possible  are  not  always  demonstrated  (208). 
No  doubt  Mallock  (i  78)  gave  far  too  optimistic  values 
of  resolution  in  spider  ocelli  since  he  used  the  out- 
moded Rayleigh  criterion  in  his  calculations.  Ho- 
mann's  estimates  (127-129)  correspond  more  closely 
with  observed  reactions. 

In.sect  behavior  seems  to  match  reasonably  well 
with  predictions  based  on  measurement  of  eyes  and 
binocular  fields  (29,  47,  172,  173,  250,  307).  Lack  of 
accommodation — an  ability  claimed  for  ommatidia 
only  once  (265) — is  of  no  significance  in  an  apposition 
eye  since  no  image  is  formed  (256),  or  in  a  superposi- 
tion eye  since  image  resolution  has  been  sacrificed  for 
increa.sed  sensitivity.  The  mosaic  style  of  vision  tends 
to  stress  the  importance  of  movement  and  find  detail 
only  at  very  close  range. 


REFERENCES 


1.  Abbott,  C.  E.    Tuiiox  .News  27:   138,   1949. 

2.  Alexandrowicz,  J.  S.  Arch.  zool.  exper.  et gen.  66:  71,  1927. 

3.  Andrews,  E.  A.  J.  Morphol.  5:  271,  1892. 

4.  Arey,  L.  B.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.  26:  359,   191 6. 

5.  Arey,  L.  B.  and  W.  J.  Crozier.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  32:  443, 
1921. 

6.  AuTRUM,  H.  Experienlia  5:  271,   1949. 

7.  AuTRUM,   H.   AND   N.   Stocker.   Bwl .   ^enlialbl.    71:    129, 
1952- 

8.  Baldus,  K.  .^Isikr.  vergleich.  Pliysiol.  3:  475,   1926. 

9.  Bauers,  C.  Z^schr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  34:  589,    1953. 

10.  Baumgardt,  E.  L.  M.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  31:  269,   1948. 

11.  Baumgartner,  H.  ^Ischr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  7:  56,   1928. 

12.  Baylor,  E.  R.  and  F.  E.  Smith.  Am.  Natural.  87:  97,  1953. 

13.  Beddard,  F.  E.   Voyage  of  H.  M.  S.  Challenger,  ^oot.  11(3, 
Part  33):   I,   1884. 

14.  Belehradek,  J.   and  J.    S.    Huxley.  J.   Exper.    Biol.    7: 

37.  1930- 

15.  Bennitt,  R.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  40;  381,   1924. 

16.  Bernard,  F.  Bull.  biol.  France  Belg.  Suppl.  23:   i,   1937. 

17.  Bertholf,  L.  M.  J.  Econ.  Entomol.  20:  521,   1927. 

18.  Bertholf,  L.  M.  J.  Agr.  Res.  43:  703,  1931. 

19.  Beuther,    E.   Silzber.   .ihhandl.   nalurforsch.    Ges.    Roslock    I  : 
I,  1926. 


20.  Bliss,  A.  F.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  26:  361,   1943. 

21.  Bliss,  .\.  F.  Biol.  Bull.  91:  220,   1946. 

22.  BoLWiG,    N.     V'tdensk.    .Meddelelser    Dansk    nalurhisl.    Foren. 
h  oebenhavn   i  og :  80,   1 946. 

23.  Bozler,  E.  ^Ischr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  3:  145,  1925. 

24.  Brand,  H.  ^Ischr.  wiss.  ^ool.   144:  363,   1933. 

25.  Brecher,  G.  y^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.    10:  497,   1929. 

26.  Brues,  C.  T.  Proc.  Am.  Acad.  Arts  Sc.  74:  281,   1941. 

27.  Brunotte,  C.  Compt.  rend.  Acad.  Sc,  Paris  106:  301,  1888. 

28.  Buck,  J.  B.  Physiol,  ^ool.   10:  45,   1937. 

29.  Burt,  E.  T.  and  W.  T.  Catton.  Nature,  London  170:  285, 

'95^- 

30.  Carriere,  J.  Qjiart.  J.  Microsc.  Sc.  24:  673,   1884. 

31.  Carriere,    J.    Die  Sehorgane  der    Thierc.    Munich:   Olten- 
bouig,  1885. 

32.  Causey,  N.  B.    Turtox  News  33:  200,   1955. 

33.  Chun,  C.   Verhandl.  deutsch.  zool.  Gesellsch.   13:  67,   1903. 

34.  Collins,  D.  L.  and  \V.  Maciiado.  J.  Econ.  Entomol.  28: 

'03.  1935- 

35.  CoNST.ANTiNEANU,    M.  J.    ^ool .   Jalirb.,    .ibt.    Anat.    Ontog. 

Tiere  52:  253,   1930. 

36.  Corneli,    W.    .^ool.   Jahrb.,    .iht.    .-inat.    Ontog.     Tiere    46: 

573.  '924- 

37.  CowLES,  R.   p.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  9:  387,    1910. 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


643 


38. 

39- 
40. 

41- 
42. 

43- 

44. 

45- 

46. 

47- 
48. 

49- 

5°- 
51- 

52- 

53- 
54- 
55- 
56. 

57- 
58. 

59- 
60. 
61. 
62. 
63- 

64. 
65- 

66. 
67. 
68. 

69. 
70. 

71- 
72. 

73- 
74- 
75- 
76. 

77- 

78. 
79- 

80. 
81. 


82. 


83- 


Crane,  J.  Z^^kgic  39:  85,   1954. 
Crozier,  W.  J.  J.  Exper.  .^00/.  24:  211,   1917. 
Crozier,  W.  J.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  2:  627,   1920. 
Crozier,  W.  J.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  3:  57,  1920. 
Crozier,  W.  J.  and  E.  Wolf.  Biol.  Bull.  77:  126,  1939. 
CuENOT,  L.  In:  Traile  de  Z^ologie,  edited  by  P.-P.  Grasse. 
Paris:  Masson,   1949,  vol.  6,  p.  46. 
Day,  M.  F.  Biol.  Bull.  80:  275,   1941. 

Demoll,  R.  Zo"^-  Jahrb.,  Abt.  Anat.  Onlog.  Tiere  27: 
651,  1909. 

Demoll,  R.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.    129:  461,    1909. 
Demoll,  R.  .^00/.  Jahrh.,  Abt.  Sysl.  28:  523,  1909. 
Demoll,  R.  Ergebn.  Fortschr.  ^ool-  2:  431,   1910. 
Demoll,  R.  Z""!-  Jahrb.,  Abl.  allg.  Z^ol-  Physiol.  30:  i6g, 
1911. 

Demoll,  R.  Biol.  Z.''"l'"lhl.  33:  727,  1913. 
Demoll,  R.   Die  Sumesorgane  der  Arthropoden,  ihr  Bau  und 
ihre  Funkiion.  Brunswick,  Germany:  Vieweg,   191 7. 
Demoll,   R.   and  L.   Scheuring.   Z.'"'^-  3<>hrb.,  Abt.  allg. 
Zool.  Physiol.  31:  519,   191 2. 

de  Serres,  M.  Phil.  Mag.  44:   107,   183,  274,   1814. 
Dethier,  V.  G.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.   19:  301,   1942. 
Dethier,  V.  G.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  22:   115,   1943. 
Dewar,  J.  and  J.  G.  M'Kendrick.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Edin- 
burgh 8:   179,   1873. 

Dietrich,  \V.  Z'"'^-  ^"Z-  32:  470,  1907. 
Dietrich,  VV.  Z'schr.  wiss.  ^ool.  92 :  465,   1 909. 
Dubois,  R.  IX  Internal,  zool.  Congres,  Monaco  1 :  148,  191 3. 
Duces,  A.  Ann.  Sc.  Nat.  Zfiol.  21:  341,   1830. 
Eggert,  B.  Zfi"^-  ^"Z-  73:  33,  1927- 

Eltringham,  H.  Trans.  Roy.  Enlomol.  Soc.  London:  i,  1919. 
Eltringham,  H.  Butler  fly  Lore.  New  York:  O.xford,  1923, 
p.  117. 

Erhardt,  a.  Biol.  Zft'i'lbl.  52:  321,   1932. 
Escher-Derivieres,  J.,  E.  Lederer  and  M.-L.  Verrier. 
Compt.  rend.  Acad.  sc.  Pans  207:  1447,   1938. 
Ewing,  W.  Edinburgh  J.  Sc.  5:  297,   1826. 
Exner,  S.  Sitzber.  Akad.  Wiss.  Wien  (,Abt.  Ill)  98:  13,  i88g. 
Exner,  S.  Die  Physiologie  der    Facettirten  Augen  von  hrebsen 
und  Insecten.  Leipzig:  Deuticke,   1891. 
Fales,  D.  E.  Biol.  Bull.  54:  534,   1928. 
Faust,  E.  C.  Bwl.  Bull.  35:   117,   1918. 
Fingerman,  M.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.   120:  131,   1952. 
Fingerman,  M.  and  F.  A.  Brown,  Jr.  Science  116:   171, 

1952- 

Fox,  H.  M.  Biol.  Rev.   i:  219,   1925. 

Fraenkel,  G.  Z^schr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  6:  385,   1927. 

Graham,  C.  H.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  2:  295,   1932. 

Graham,  C.  H.  and  H.  K.  Hartline.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  18: 

9'7.  1935- 

Granit,   R.   Receptors  and  Sensory  Perception.   New   Haven: 

Yale  Univ.  Press,   1955. 

Grave,  C.  and  G.  Rilev.  J.  Morphol.  57:  185,  1935. 
Greeff,  R.  Sitzber.   Gesellsch.   Be/order,  ges.  Nalurw.   Alar- 
burg:  115,  1875. 

Greeff,  R.  Nova  Acta  Leopoldina  39:  33,  1877. 
Grenacher,    G.    H.    Untersuchungen    iiber   das   Sehorgan   der 
Arthropoden,  insbesondere  der  Spinnen,  Insekten  und  Crustaceen. 
Gottingen:  Vanderliock  u.  Ruprecht,  1879. 
Hanstrom,  B.  Kgl.  Svenska  Vetenskapsakad.  Handl.  [3]  4:  I, 
1926. 

Hanstrom,  B.  Arch.  Entwicklungsmechn.  Organ.  115:  154, 
1929. 


84. 

85- 

86. 

87. 
88. 
89. 
go. 

9>- 
92- 
93- 
94- 
95- 
96. 
97- 


99- 
100. 

101. 
102. 
103. 
104. 
105. 
106. 
107. 
108. 
log. 

1 10. 
III. 
1 12. 

'■3- 
114. 

'■5- 
116. 
117, 
118. 
119. 
120. 
121. 
122. 


123. 
124. 

'25- 
126. 
127. 
128. 
129. 
130. 
131- 

132. 
'33- 

'34- 


Hanstrom,  B.  Lunds  Univ.  Arsskr.  N.  F.  30:  i,  1934. 
Harper,  E.  H.  Biol.  Bull.  10:  17,  1905. 
Harrington,  N.  R.  and  E.  Leaming.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  3: 
9,  i8gg. 

Hartline,  H.  K.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  83:  466,  1928. 
Hartline,  H.  K.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  11 ;  465,  1938. 
H.\rtline,  H.  K.  The  Harvey  Lectures  Ser.  37:  39,  1941. 
Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  E.  F.  MacNichol, 
Jr.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symp.  Qiiant.  Biol.  17:  125,  1952. 
Haug,  G.  <^/,(fAr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  19:  246,  1933. 
Heath,  H.  Proc.  Acad.  .Nat.  Sc.  Philadelphia  56:  257,  1904. 
Hecht,  S.  J.  Exper.  ^ool-  25:  261,  1918. 
Hecht,  S.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  3:  367,  1921. 
Hecht,  S.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  3:  375,  1921. 
Hecht,  S.  Science  53:  347,   192 1. 
Hecht,  S.  Am.  Scientist  32;  159,   1944. 
Hecht,  S.  and  G.  Wald.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sc,  Washington 
19:  964,  1933. 

Hecht,  S.  and  G.  Wald.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  17:  517,  1934. 
Heidermanns,  C.  .^00/.  Jahrb.,  Abt.  allg.  Zfiol-  Physiol.  45: 
6og,  1928. 

Heine,  L.  Med.  nalurwiss.  Arch,  i:  322,  1907. 
Hensen,  V.  Z^schr.  wiss.  Zool.  15:  155,  1865. 
Hensen,  V.  Zool-  Anz.  i :  30,  1878. 
Hertel,  E.  ^te/ir.  allg.  Physiol.  4:  i,  1904. 
Herter,  K.  .^toAr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  4:  103,   ig26. 
Hess,  C.  <^««/ra/A/.  Physiol.  16:  91,  1902. 
Hess,  C.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  172:  449,  1918. 
Hess,  C.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  181:  i,  1920. 
Hess,  C.  and  A.  Gerwerzhagen.  Arch,  vergleich.  Ophthal- 
mol. 4:  300,  19 1 4. 

Hess,  W.  N.  J.  Morphol.  Physiol.  41  :  63,  1925. 
Hess,  W.  N.  Carnegie  Inst.  Washington  Tr.  Bk.  30:382,  1931. 
Hess,  W.  N.  J.  Exper.  Zool-  79:  i,  1938. 
Hess,  W.N.Carnegie  Inst.  Washington  Publ.  517:  153,  1940. 
Hesse,  R.  .^(.tcAr.  wiss.  Zool-  61 :  693,  1896. 
Hesse,  R.  Z'^''^"'-  wiss.  Zool.  62:  527,  1897. 
Hesse,  R.  .^/jc/ir.  wiss.  Zool-  63:  456,  1898. 
Hesse,  R.  <^/.icAr.  wiss.  Zool.  65:  446,  i8g9. 
Hesse,  R.  Zool.  Anz.  24:  30,  1901. 
Hesse,  R.  .^/ic/ir.  wiss.  Zool.  70:  347,  1901. 
Hesse,  R.  .^/icAr.  wiss.  Zool.  72:  565,  1902. 
Hesse,  R.  Das  Sehen  niederer  Tiere.  Jena:  Fischer,  1908. 
Hevm.an,  C.  and  A.  R.  Moore.  J.  Gen,  Physiol.  7:  345, 

1925- 

Hilton,  W.  A.  J.  Enlomol.  Zool.  13(3):  49,  1921. 

Hilton,  W.  A.  J.  Entomol.  Zool.  13(4):  55,  ig2i. 

Hilton,  W.  A.  J.  Enlomol.  Zool.  16(3):  8g,  1924. 

Hom.ann,  H.  Zl'cf"'-  vergleich.  Physiol,  i:  541,  1924. 

Hom.\nn,  H.  Zl^'^l"'-  vergleich.  Physiol.  7:  201,  1928. 

Hom.ann,  H.  .^/jcAr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  14:  40,  1931. 

Homann,  H.  .^/.(cAr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  20:  420,  1934. 

Homann,  H.  <|'te/;r.  Naturforsch.  b2 :  161,  1947. 

Hundertmark,    a.   Zl^'^l"-    Forst-   u.   Jagdwesen    70:    225, 

1938. 

Hyman,  L.  H.   The  Invertebrates:  Prolo.zoa  through  Clenophora. 

New  York:  McGraw-Hill,    1940. 

Hyman,    L.    H.     The   Invertebrates:    II.    Platyhelminlhes    and 

Rhynchocoela;  the  Acoelomale  Bilaleria.  New  York:  McGraw- 

Hill,  1951. 

H^'MAN,  L.  H.  The  Invertebrates :  III.  Acanthocephala,  Aschel- 

minthes,   and  Entoprocta;   the   Pseudocoelomate   Bilateria.    New 

York:  McGraw-Hill,    ig5i. 


644 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGV 


iNEUROPHVSIOLOGY    I 


135- 

136, 

137- 
.38. 

139- 
140. 

141. 
142. 

143- 
144. 

145- 

.46. 
147. 


149- 

150. 

151- 
152- 
'53- 

■54- 

155- 
.56. 

137- 

.58. 
'59- 

160. 
161. 
162. 
163, 

164. 
165. 
166. 
167. 
168. 
169. 
170. 
171. 
172. 

'73- 
174. 

175- 
176. 
177. 

178. 

'79- 
180. 
181. 
182. 
183. 
.84. 


Hyman,    L.    H.     The    Invertebrates:   IV.    Echinodermata ;    the 

Coelomate  Bilateria.  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,   1955. 

Imhof,  O.  E.  Biol,  ^entralbl.  21  :  189,  1901. 

Imhof,  O.  E.  Biol,  ^entralbl.  21:  459,  1901. 

IsEERG,  O.  Geol.  Foren.  i  Stockholm  Fork.  39:  593,  191 7. 

Janda,  V.  ^ool.  Anz-  96:  77,  1931. 

Janzen,  R.  .^Ischr.  Morphol.  Okol.  Tiere  24:  327,  1932. 

Joseph,  H.  Biol.  Generalis  4:  237,   1928. 

Kahmann,  H.  Tabul.  biol.  Q's-Grav.')  22:  i,  1947. 

Kampa,  E.  M.  Nature,  London    175:  996,   1955. 

Kepner,  W.  a.  and  a.  M.  Foshee.  J.  E.xper.  Physiol.  23; 

519.  '9'7- 

Kiesel,  a.  Sit-ber.  Akad.    U'iss.    Wien  (^Abt.  III^   103:  97, 

1894. 

Kleinholz,  L.  H.  Biol.  Bull.  109:  362,  1955. 

Kleinholz,   L.   H.    and   W.   Henwood.   Anal.   Rec.    117: 

637.  '953- 

KoLLER,    G.    AND    G.    VON    Studnitz.    ^tschr .    lergleich. 

Physiol.  20:  388,  1934. 

KosswiG,    C.    AND    L.    KossvviG.    Verhandl.    deutsch.    ^ool. 

Gesellsch.  38:  274,  1936. 

Krause,  a.  C.   Tabul.  biol.  i^s-Grav.')  22:  200,   1951. 

Krohn,  a.  j\'ova  Acta  Leopoldina  19:  41,  1842. 

Krugelis-Macrae,  E.  Biol.  Bull.  110:  69,  1956. 

Krukenberg,  C.  F.  W.  Unters.  physiol.  Inst.  Heidelb.  2:1, 

1882. 

KiJHN,  A.  Nachr.  Ges.   Wiss.  Gottingen.  Jahresber.   Geschdjts- 

jahr.  Math,  physik.  KL:  66,  1923. 

KiJHN,  A.  Naturwissenschaften  12:  116,   1924. 

KiJHN,  A.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  5:  762,  1927. 

KiJHN,  A.  AND  G.  Fraenkel.  .Nachr.  Ges.   Wiss.  Gottingen. 

Jahresber.  Geschdftsjahr.  Math,  physik.  KL:  330,  1927. 

KiJHN,  A.  AND  R.  PoHL.  Naturwissemchajteu  9:  738,   1921. 

KuPFER,    M.    Vierteljahresschr.    naturforsch.    Ges.  ^iirich  60: 

568,  igis- 

Landois,  H.  ^tschr.  wiss.^ool.  16:  27,  1866. 
Langdon,  F.  E.  J.  Morphol.  11:  193,   1895. 
Langdon,  F.  E.  J.  Comp.  .\eurol.  10:  i,   1900. 
Langeloh,    H.    p.   .^ool.   Jahrb.,    .ibt.    nltg.    ^onl.    Physiol. 
57:  235,  1937. 

Langer,  C.  Sitzber.  Akad.  Wiss.  Wien(^Abt.  7)5:324,  1850. 
Lemke,  G.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  22:  298,  1935. 
Light,  V.  E.  J.  Morphol.  Physiol.  49:  i,  1930. 
Link,  E.  Verhandl.  deutsch.  zool.  Gesellsch.  18:  161,   1908. 
Link,  E.  ^ool.  Anz-  33:  445,  1908. 

hiNK,  IL.  ^ool.Jalirb.,Abt.  Anat.  Ontog.  T/pre  27;  213,  1909. 
Link,  E.  ,^00/.  J'aAri.,  .Abt.Anat.  Ontog.  Tiere  2y  -.281,  1909. 
LocHHEAD,  J.  H.  Anat.  Rec.  75,  Suppl.:  64,   1939. 
Luedtke,  H.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  26:  162,   1938. 
Luedtke,  H.  Z^schr.  Morphol.  Okol.  Tiere  37:  i,   1940. 
LuTZ,  F.  E.  Ann.  New  York  Acad.  Sc.  29;  181,  1924. 
LuTZ,  F.  E.  Am.  Museum  Novitates  641:  i,  1933. 
LuTZ,  F.  E.  Natural  History  33:  565,   1933. 
MacNichol,  E.  F.,  H.  H.  Wagner  and  H.  K.  Hart- 
line.  A7.Y  Internal  Physiol.  Congr.,  Proc:  582,   1953. 
Mallock,  a.  Nature,  London  113:  45,  1924. 
Marcus,  E.  Biol,  tiere  Deutsch.  14:  i,  1925. 
Mast,  S.  O.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  20:  i,  1916. 
Mast,  S.  O.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  34:  149,  1921. 
Mast,  S.  O.  Arch.  Prolistenk.  60:  197,  1928. 
Mast,  S.  O.  and  N.  Stahler.  Biol.  Bull.  73:  126,  1937. 
Mayer,  A.  G.  and  C.  G.  Soule.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  3:  415, 
1906. 


.85. 
186. 

.87. 


189. 
190. 
191. 
192. 

193- 


194- 

195- 
196. 

'97- 
198. 

■99- 
200. 
201. 


203. 
204. 
205. 
206. 
207. 
208. 
209. 
210. 
211. 
212. 

213- 
214. 

215- 
216. 


218. 
219. 
220. 
221. 
222. 

223. 
224. 
225. 

226. 
227. 
228. 
229. 

230. 

231. 

232. 
•^33- 


Merlin,  D.  ^ool.  Bidrag  Uppsala  8:  i,  1923. 

Menzer,    G.    and    K.    Stockhammer.   .Xaturwissenschaften 

38:  190.  1951  • 

Merker,    E.    and    H.    Gilbert,   ^ool.   Jahrb.,   Aht.    allg. 

^ool.  Physiol.  50:  479,   1932. 

MiLLOTT,  N.  Biol.  Bull.  99:  329,  1950. 

Millott,  N.  Nature,  London  171:  973,   1953. 

MiLLOTT,  N.  Tr.  Royal  Soc,  London  ser.  B  238:   187,    1954. 

Millott,  N.  Endeavour  16:  19,  1957. 

Millott,    N.     and   H.   G.   Vevers.    J.    marine   Biol.    .A. 

U.K.  34:  279,  1955. 

Milne,   L.  J.   and   M.  J.   Milne.    In:   Radiation   Biology, 

edited  by  A.  HoUaender.  New  York ;  McGraw-Hill,  1 956, 

vol.  3,  p.  621. 

MiNKiEWicz,    R.    Compt.   rend.   Acad,   sc,   Paris    143:   785, 

1906. 

MiNKiEWicz,  R.  Conipt.  rend.  Acad,  sc,  Paris  143:934,  1906. 

Mitsukuri,  K.  Annot.  ^ool.  Japon.  4:  i,  1901. 

Moore,  A.  R.  Arch.  sc.  biol.  8;  112,  1926. 

MosELEY,  H.  N.  Quart.  J.  Microsc.  Sc.  25:  37,   1885. 

Mosell.'\,  R.  G.  Boll.  1st.  zool.  Univ.  Roma  4:  166,  1927. 

MuRBACH,  L  Biol.  Bull.  14:  I,  1907. 

Nagel,  \V.  .\.  Der  Lichtsinn  .Augenloser  7"?Vrf.  Jena :  Fischer, 

1896. 

Notth.aft,   J.  Abh.  senckenberg.  naturforsch,  Gesellsch.  12:  35, 

1881. 

Olmsted,  J.  M.  D.  J.  Exper.  J^ool.  24:  333,  1917. 

P.arker,  G.  H.  Proc  .SV.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  3:  61,  1906. 

Parker,  G.  H.  Proc.  Am.  Phtlos.  Soc.  61  ;  107,  1922. 

Patten,  \V.  .Mitt.  zool.  Stat.  .Veapel  6:  542,  1886. 

Perkins,  E.  B.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  50:  71,  1928. 

Petrunkevitch,  a.  J.  E.xper.  ^ool.  5:  275,   1907. 

Pflug feeder,  O.  Z""!-  '^"Z-  89:  276,  1930. 

Pflug feeder,  O.  ^tschr.  wiss.^ool.  142:  540,  1932. 

Piper,  H.  Arch.  Anat.  Physiol.,  Physiol.  Abl.:  453,   1904. 

Piper,  H.  Arch.  .4nat.  Physiol.,  Physiol.  Abt.:  85,   191 1. 

PiRENNE,  M.  H.  .\ature,  London   170;  824,    1952. 

Plate,  L.  H.  ^ooI.  Jahrb.  4,  Suppl.:  i,  1897. 

Plate,  L.  H.  ^ooI.  Jahrb.  5;  15,  281,  1899. 

Plate,  L.  H.  Allgemeine  ^oologie  und  Abstammungslehre.  II. 

Die  Sinnesorgane  der  Tiere.  Jena:  Fischer,  1924. 

Plateau,  F.  Bull.  acad.  roy.  Belg.  14:  545,  1887. 

Prosser,  C.  L.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  4:  363,  1934. 

Rab.\ud,  E.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,  sc,  Paris  173:  606,  1921. 

R.AB.AUD,  E.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  92:  603,  1925. 

R.adl,  E.  ^ool.  ^entralbl.  9:  82,  1902. 

Ramon  y  Caj.al,  S.   Trab.  Lab.  Invest.  Biol.  Univ.  Madrid 

7:217,  1909. 

Rawitz,  B.  Arch.  Anat.  Physiol.,  Physiol.  Abt.:  367,  1891. 

Redikorzew,  W.  .^tschr.  wiss.  Zool.  68:  581,   1900. 

Richards,  M.  H.  and  E.  Y.  Furrow.  Biol.  Bull.  48:  243, 

'925- 

Richter,  R.  ^entralbl.  Mineral.  Geol.:  344,  1922. 

RoESCH,  P.  Jena,  ^tschr.  Naturwiss.  5o(N.F.  43):  97,  1913. 

Rulher,  F.  Bull.  lab.  maritime  Dinard  30:  21,  1948. 

St.  George,  R.  C.  C.  and  G.  Wald.  Biol.  Bull.  97 :  248, 

'949- 

Sanchez,  D.  S.  y.  .irch.  .\'eurobiol.  3:  337,   1922. 

Sanchez,  D.   S.   y.    Trab.  Lab.   Invest.   Biol.   Univ.  Madrid 

21:  143,  1923. 

Schallek,  VV.  J.  E.xper.  Zool-  91 :  155,  1942. 

ScHLICHER,  J.   Verhandl.  naturh.   Ver.  preuss.  Rheinl.   Westjal. 

82:  197,  1926. 


PHOTOSENSITIVITY    IN    INVERTEBRATES 


645 


234.  ScHLUTER,  E.  ^tschr.  wiss.^iiiil.   143:  538,  1933. 

235.  ScHMiTT-AuRACHER,  A.  Biol .  ^eniratbl.  43:  225,   1923. 

236.  ScHONE,  H.  ^tschr.  verglnch.  Physiol.  33;  63,  1951. 

237.  ScHREINER,  K.  E.  Bergem  Museums  Arbok,  .Xu/urv.  1897:  i, 
1898. 

238.  Shettles,  L.  B.  J.  Exper.  ^ool.  -j-j:  215,  1937. 

239.  Shortess,  G.  S.  Physiol,  ^ool.  15:  184,  1942. 

240.  Smith,   F.   E.   and   E.   R.   Baylor.   Am.  .\atural.  87 :  49, 

1953- 

241 .  Smith,  G.  Bull.  Museum  Comp.  Zool.  Harvard 48 :  233,  1 906. 

242.  Smith,  J.  E.  Phil.  Trans.  B227:  iii,  1937. 

243.  Spengel,  J.  \V.  Fauna  e  Flora  Golf.  Xapoli  18:  i,  1893. 

244.  Stephens,  G.  C,  M.  Fingerman  and  F.  A.  Brown,  Jr. 
.4nn.  Entomol.  Soc.  Am.  46;  75,   1953. 

245.  Stiasnv,  G.  y^tschr.  wiss.^ool.   iio:  36,  1914 

246.  Strohm,  K.  ^ool.  Anz-  36:  156,  1910. 

247.  Stschegolew,  G.  G.  Rev.  tool,  russe  7;  149,  1927. 

248.  SzczAWiNSKA,  W.  Arch.  Biol.  Paris  10:  523,  1890. 

249.  Taliaferro,  W.  H.  J.  Exper.  Z""^-  3'  •  59-  1920- 

250.  Tischler,   W.   Z""^-  Johrb.,   .-ibi.   allg.   -(^ool.    Physiol.    57: 

'57.  1936- 

251.  ToMPSETT,  D.  H.  Liverpool  .Marine  Biol.  Comm.  .Mem.  32; 

1.  1939- 

252.  TiJMPEL,  R.  ^tschr.  wiss.  Insekienbiol.  8;   167,  218,   1912. 

253.  TiJMPEL,  R.  ^Ischr.  wiss.  Insekienbiol.   10:  275,   1914. 

254.  UcHiDA,  H.  J.   Fac.   Sc.   Imp.    Univ.    Tokyo  Sect.    IV,   3: 

517.  ■934- 

255.  Unteutsch,  W.  ^ool.  Jahrb.,  Abl.  allg.  ^ool.  Physiol.  58: 

69.  1937- 

256.  VAN  der  Horst,  C.  J.  Acta  ^ool.  14;  loi,  1933. 

257.  VAN  Herwerden,  M.  a.  Biol.  Z^ntralbl.  34:  213,  1914. 

258.  VAN  Weel,  p.  B.  Arch,  neerl.  ^ool.  i :  347,  1935. 

259.  VAN  Weel,  P.  B.  and  S.  Thore.  ^tschr.  vergleich.  Physiol. 
23:  26,  1936. 

260.  ViAUD,  G.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  129:  11 74,  1 938. 

261.  ViAUD,  G.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  129:  1177,  1938. 

262.  ViAUD,  G.  Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.  129;  11 78,  1938. 

263.  ViAUD,  G.  Bull.  biol.  France  Belg.  74;  249,  1940. 

264.  ViAUD,  G.  Bull.  biol.  France  Belg.  77:  68,  1943. 

265.  ViGiER,  P.  Compt.  rend.  Acad,  sc,  Paris  138:  775,  1904. 

266.  VON  Apathy,  S.    Verhandl.  intern,  .zool.  Kongr.    Berlin   1901: 
707,  1902. 

267.  VON  BuDDENBROCK,  W.  Der  Biologe  3;  231,  1934. 

268.  VON  BuDDENBROCK,  W.  Biol.  Rev.  10;  283,  1935. 

269.  VON   BuDDENBROCK,    \V.    Vergleichende   Physiologie.    Band  I: 
Sinnesphysiologie.  Basel:  Birkhauser,    1952. 

270.  VON  Frisch,  K.  ^ool.  Jahrb.,  Aht.  allg.  ^ool.   Physiol.   35: 
I,  1914. 

271.  VON  Frisch,  K.  Experientia  5:  142,  1949. 

272.  VON  Frisch,  K..  Experientia  6:  210,  1950. 


273.  VON    Frisch,    K.    Bees:   their    Vision,    Chemical   Senses,    and 
Language.  Ithaca;  Cornell  Univ.  Press,   1950. 

274.  VON  Pflugk.  Ber.  ophthal.  Gesellsch.  36:  54,   1910. 

275.  VON  Uxkull,  J.  ^tschr.  Biol.  34:  319,  1897. 

276.  Wald,  G.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  479,  1941. 

277.  Wald,  G.  Am.  Scientist  42:  72,  1954. 

278.  Watase,   S.  Stud.   Biol.   Lab.  Johns  Hopkins   L'niv.   4:   287, 
1890. 

279.  Waterman,  T.  H.  Science  11 1:  252,   1950. 

280.  Waterman, T.H.  Tr.  New  fork  Acad.  Sc.    Q2)  14:11,  1951. 

281.  Waterman,   T.   H.   Proc.  Mat.  Acad.   Sc,    Washington  39: 
287,  1953. 

282.  W.aterman,   T.    H.    Proc.   Aat.   Acad.   Sc,    Washington   40: 

252.  1954- 

283.  Waterman,  T.   H.   Proc.  .\at.   Acad.  Sc,    Washington  40: 

258,  1954- 

284.  Waterman,  T.  H.  J.  Morphol.  95:  125,  1954. 

285.  Waterman,  T.  H.  Science  120:  927,  1954. 

286.  Waterman,  T.   H.   and  C.   A.   G.   Wiersma.  J.   Exper. 
Zool.  126:  59,   1954. 

287.  Weber,  H.  Z.""^-  -^"-i-  '°8:  49,  1934- 

288.  Weiss,  H.  B.  J.  Econ.  Entomol.  36:  i,  1943. 

289.  Welsh,  J.  H.  J.  Exper.  Z""!-  .56:  459,  1930. 

290.  Welsh,  J.  H.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  4:  379,  1934. 

291.  Wenrich,  D.  H.  Brit.  J.  Anim.  Behav.  6:  297,   191 6. 

292.  Werner,  O.  Z""!-  Jahrb.,  Abt.  allg.  Z^ol.  Physiol.  43:  41, 
1926. 

293.  Whitman,  C.  O.  Z.'"'l-  Jahrb.,  Abt.  Anal.  Ontog.    Here  6; 
616,  1893. 

294.  Widmann,  E.  Z^schr.  wiss.  Z^ol.  90:  258,  1908. 

295.  WiLLEM,  V.  Arch.  Biol.  Paris  12:  57,  1892. 

296.  Wilson,  N.   Tr.  Linnean  Soc.  London  23:  107,  i860. 

297.  Wolf,  E.  Z^schr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  3:  209,   1925. 

298.  Wolf,  E.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  16:  407,  1933. 

299.  Wolf,  E.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  16:  773,  1933. 

300.  Wolf,   E.   and  G.   Zerrahn-Wolf.  J.   Gen.   Physiol.    r8: 

853.  1935- 

301.  Wolf,   E.   and  G.   Zerrahn-Wolf.  J.   Gen.   Physiol.   20: 

767.  '937- 

302.  WOLSKY,   A.   Z""^-    ^"^^    80:   56,    1929. 

303.  WoLSKV,  A.  Z^schr.  vergleich.  Physiol.  12:  783,  1930. 

304.  WoLSKY,  A.  ^I'/icAr.  vergleich.  Physiol.   14:  385,  193 1. 

305.  Yonge,  C.  M.  Sc.  Rep.  Brit.  Museum  1:  283,  1936. 

306.  Yung,  E.  Arch.  sc.  phys.  et  nat.  118:  77,  1913. 

307.  Zacharias,  O.  Monatschr.  Mitt.  Gesammtgeb.  .Naturwiss.  7: 
173,  1890. 

308.  Z.AVREL,  J.  .^00/.  .Inc.  31  :  247,  1907. 

309.  Zimmermann,  K.  Zo^l-  Jahrb.,  Abt.  Anat.  Ontog.   Tiere  37; 
I.  1913- 


CHAPTER   XXVII 


The  image-forming  mechanism  of  the  eye 


GLENN  A.   FRY     |     School  of  Optometry,  The  Ohio  State  University,  Colimhus,  Ohio 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Image  Formation 

GuUstrand's  Schematic  Eye  and  Its  Refracting  Mechanism 

Formation  of  Image  by  Refracting  Mechanism 

Refracting  Power  of  the  Eye 

Helmholtz  Schematic  Eye 

Reduced  Eye 

Role  of  Pupil  in  Image  Formation 

Chief  Rays 

Blur  Circles  in  Eye  Free  from  Astigmatism 

Astigmatism 

Lines  of  Sight 

Primary  Line  of  Sight  and  Foveal  Chief  Ray 

Pupillary  Axis  and  Angle  X 

Size  of  Retinal  Image 
Refraction  and  Accommodation  of  the  Eye 

Refraction  of  the  Eye 

Accommodation 

Static  Refraction  of  the  Eye 

Correction  for  Ametropia 

Specification  of  Amount  of  Accommodation  in  Play 

Aphakia 
Ocular  Measurements 

Indices  of  Media 

Purkinje  Images 

Optic  Axis  of  the  Eye 

Configuration  of  Front  Surface  of  Cornea 

Measurement  of  Internal  Refracting  Surfaces 

X-ray  Measurement  of  Axial  Length  of  the  Eye 

X-ray  Determination  of  Location  of  Second  Nodal  Point 

Locating  Conjugate  Focus  of  Retina 
Mechanism  of  Accommodation 

Intraocular  Mechanism  of  Accommodation 

Ciliary  Muscle  Potential 

Innervation  Controlling  Accommodation 

Night  and  Sky  Myopia 
Visual  Field 
Retinal  Illuminance 

Light  and  Illuminance 

Solid  Angle 

Luminance 

Retinal  Illuminance 

Transmittance  of  the  Eye 


Stiles-Crawford  Effect 
Stray  Light  in  the  Eye 
Blur  of  Retinal  Image 
Entoptic  Phenomena 


THE  EYE  PLAYS  THE  TRIPLE  ROLE  of  gathering  infor- 
mation, coding  it  and  relaying  it  to  the  brain.  In 
this  chapter  we  are  concerned  only  with  the  role  which 
the  eye  plays  as  an  optical  device  in  gathering  in- 
formation. 

In  trying  to  explain  this  role  I  have  started  with  a 
schematic  e\e  which  is  free  from  some  of  the  defects 
and  complications  of  an  actual  eye.  With  this  kind  of 
an  eye  one  can  explain  how  an  image  is  formed  and 
what  is  meant  by  refracting  power,  refraction  of  the 
eye,  size  of  the  retinal  image,  etc. 

The  eye  is  not  like  a  telescope  which  can  be  taken 
apart  to  find  out  how  it  works.  Hence  it  is  necessary 
to  develop  approaches  which  are  not  needed  with  an 
ordinary  optical  device.  For  example,  the  focal  length 
of  an  eye  cannot  be  measured  directly  and  we  have  to 
substitute  the  concept  of  refraction  to  provide  an 
index  of  an  eye's  performance  as  an  optical  instru- 
ment. An  attempt  will  be  made  to  explain  how  this 
and  other  measurements  are  made  on  a  living  eye. 
After  explaining  these  basic  concepts  and  methods  of 
measurement,  consideration  will  be  given  to  the 
mechanism  whereby  the  eye  can  change  its  focus. 

The  physiologist  is  by  right  inore  concerned  with 
the  response  of  the  retina  to  light  than  the  mechanics 
of  applying  light  to  the  retina,  but  there  are  some 
special  problems  that  arise  in  describing  the  stimulus 
applied  to  the  retina  to  which  attention  must  be 
given.  In  optics  the  word  illuminance  is  used  to  de- 
scribe the  rate  at  which  light  is  applied  to  the  retina, 
but  the  physiologist  wants  to  call  this  stimulus  inten- 


647 


648 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHVSUJLOGV 


NEUROPHVSIOLOGY    I 


sity.  This  is  confusing  because  in  optics  the  term 
intensity  is  reserved  to  designate  the  candlepower  of  a 
point  source. 

Furthermore,  blur  produces  a  pattern  of  illumi- 
nance on  the  retina  which  is  quite  different  from  the 
distribution  of  luminance  in  the  visual  field,  and  in 
most  cases  it  is  the  blur  inherent  in  the  image- 
forming  mechanism  and  not  the  structure  of  the 
retina  which  limits  the  ability  of  the  eye  to  resolve 
fine  detail. 

Stray  light  in  the  eye  also  presents  a  proljlem.  Al- 
though the  stray  light  is  feeble  in  comparison  with 
the  focused  light  which  is  applied  to  a  small  spot  on 
the  retina,  it  still  has  to  be  reckoned  with  in  relating 
the  light  response  of  the  pupil  and  the  potential  of 
the  electroretinogram  to  the  pattern  of  stimulation 
applied  to  the  retina.  We  are  dealing  not  only  with 
the  small  number  of  photoreceptors  responding  to 
focused  light  but  also  with  the  millions  of  photo- 
receptors responding  to  stray  light. 

This  chapter  also  includes  a  section  on  entoptic 
phenomena  because  they  are  used  in  various  indirect 
ways  to  help  us  understand  how  the  eye  gathers  in- 
formation. 

The  study  of  the  image-forming  mechanism  of  the 
eyes  has  a  long  history  because  as  soon  as  man  began 
to  think  about  himself  as  something  separate  from 
the  external  world,  he  assumed  the  reality  of  the 
external  world  and  began  to  wonder  how  he  could 
.see  external  objects.  At  first  he  supposed  that  images 
were  given  off  by  objects  and  transmitted  into  the 
eye.  He  reasoned  that  these  images  must  be  reduced 
in  size  in  order  to  get  through  the  pupil.  The  discovery 
of  the  small  images  reflected  by  the  cornea  led  to  the 
belief  that  these  images  are  responsible  for  \'ision,  and 
the  lens  and  not  the  retina  was  assumed  to  be  the 
structure  assigned  to  relay  the  images  to  the  brain. 
This  view  lasted  for  centuries.  About  the  beginning  of 
the  seventeenth  century,  Kepler  (79,  p.  116)  dis- 
covered and  described  how  an  image  is  formed  by  a 
refracting  surface.  He  then  applied  his  concepts  to 
the  eye  to  show  how  the  refracting  mechanism  of 
the  eye  must  form  an  upside-down  picture  on  the 
retina.  The  pinhole  camera  which  was  invented  about 
the  same  time  helped  to  demonstrate  how  an  upside- 
down  image  could  be  formed,  and  finally  Scheiner 
(79,  p.  116)  demonstrated  the  upside-down  image 
on  the  back  of  an  excised  eye. 

.•\ijout  this  time  attention  was  turned  away  from 
the  nature  of  the  image  on  the  retina  to  the  mecha- 
nism of  accommodation  by  which  the  eye  can  change 
its  focus.  From  the  time  of  Kepler  to  that  of  Young 


C79>  P-  158)  various  mechanisms  were  proposed  in- 
cluding change  in  length  of  the  eye,  change  in  the 
curvature  of  the  cornea,  change  in  the  position  and 
shape  of  the  lens  and  change  in  the  size  of  the  pupil. 
Young  (86,  p.  201;  79,  p.  158)  with  a  series  of  bril- 
liant experiments  at  the  beginning  of  the  nineteenth 
century  showed  that  the  lens  provides  the  basis  for  ac- 
commodation. Since  then  steady  progress  has  been 
made  in  understanding  the  various  aspects  of  image 
formation  by  the  eye. 

Helmholtz  (79)  has  presented  at  the  end  of  each  of 
his  chapters  an  historical  summary  and  a  biljli- 
ography  which  is  useful  to  those  interested  in  the 
early  history  of  the  subject.  There  are  other  general 
references  that  pertain  to  the  early  history  (74,  77,  78). 


IMAGE     FORMATION 

Giillstriind' s  Schematit  Eve  and  Its  Refracting  Mechanism 

In  demonstrating  the  principles  of  image  forma- 
tion by  the  eye,  it  is  customary  to  substitute  for  an 


/y_J. CILIARY   BODY      NX 

CORNEA    y^ 

K 

\V-RETINA 

*"            if 

lr\if' LENS   CORTEX 

^       OPTIC 

^ 

\yL               VITREOUS 

Axis 

AQUEOUSVvJ 

wC\ 

IRIS 

\\        LENS    NUCLEUS        / 

7 

FIG.  I.  Gullstrand's  schematic  eye,  in  which  the  dimen- 
sions and  indices  are  as  follows: 

mm 

Thickness  of  cornea 0.5 

Displacement  of  front   surface  of  lens  behind   front 

surface  of  cornea  3.6 

Displacement  of  nucleus  from  front  surfaceof  lens   .  .  0.546 

Thickness  of  nucleus 2.419 

Thickness  of  lens   3.6 

Index  of  refraction  of  cornea i  .  376 

Index  of  aqueous  and  vitreous i  .336 

Index  of  lens  cortex 1  .386 

Index  of  lens  nucleus i  .406 

Radius  of  front  surface  of  cornea   7.7 

Radius  of  back  surface  of  cornea 6.8 

Radius  of  front  surface  of  lens lO.o 

Radius  of  front  surface  of  nucleus 7  •9' ' 

Radius  of  back  surface  of  nucleus ~5-7^ 

Radius  of  back  surface  of  lens —6.0 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EVE 


649 


actual  eye  a  schematic  eye  such  as  Gullstrand's  (79, 
p.  392)  which  is  illustrated  in  figure  i . 

The  front  and  back  surfaces  of  the  cornea,  the 
front  and  back  surfaces  of  the  lens  and  the  front  and 
back  surfaces  of  the  nucleus  of  the  lens  are  the  refract- 
ing surfaces  and  constitute  the  refracting  mechanism. 
The  spaces  bounded  by  these  surfaces  are  assumed 
to  be  filled  with  homogeneous  transparent  media, 
but  differ  from  each  other  in  having  different  indices 
of  refraction.  The  si.\  refracting  surfaces  of  Gull- 
strand's  .schematic  eye  are  assumed  to  be  spherical 
and  centered  on  a  common  optic  a.xis. 

Ffirmation  of  Image  by  Refracting  Meehanism 

The  refracting  mechanism  forms  images  of  objects 
placed  in  front  of  the  eye.  The  simplest  kind  of  object 
that  can  be  .so  placed  is  a  monochromatic  point 
source  of  light  as  shown  in  figure  2.  The  point  source 
Q.  gives  off  rays  which  are  incident  at  the  cornea. 
Because  these  rays  diverge  from  the  point  Q_  this 
point  represents  the  focus  of  the  incident  rays.  These 
rays  are  said  to  exist  in  object  space  and  the  point 
Q.  is  called  an  object  point. 

The  pupil  admits  into  the  eye  a  certain  number  of 
the  rays  diverging  fron  the  point  Q_,  and  after  these 
rays  emerge  into  the  vitreous  they  are  said  to  exist  in 
image  space.  They  converge  at  the  point  ()'  which 
represents  the  image  point  which  is  conjugate  to  Q_. 

One  can  locate  the  image  point  corresponding  to 
a  given  object  point  by  tracing  two  or  more  rays 
through  the  refracting  surfaces.  Each  ray  entering 
the  eye  is  refracted  or  bent  at  each  surface  in  accord- 
ance with  .Snell's  law  of  refraction,  illustrated  in  figure 
3.  The  ray  in  the  first  medium  which  is  incident  to 
the  refracting  surface  makes  an  angle  a  with  the 
normal  to  the  refracting  surface  at  the  point  of  inci- 
dence. After  it  emerges  into  the  second  medium  as 
the  refracted  ray,  it  makes  an  angle  a  with  the 
normal.  .Snell's  law  states  that 

n  sin  a  =  n'  sin  a' 


where  n  represents  the  inde.x  of  refraction  of  the  first 
medium  and  n'  the  index  of  the  second  medium. 

This  method  of  locating  an  image  point  which  in- 
volves tracing  rays  from  surface  to  surface  is  tedious, 
and  it  is  much  simpler  to  locate  first  the  so-called 
cardinal  points  and  planes  and  then  use  these  to 
locate  the  image  point.  For  Gullstrand's  schematic 
eye  one  may  compute  for  any  given  wavelength  a 
pair  of  nodal  points  N  and  N' ,  a  pair  of  principal 
points  H  and  H'  and  planes,  and  a  pair  of  focal 
points  F  and  F'  and  planes  (.see  fig.  4).  The  signifi- 
cance of  these  points  and  planes  will  become  obvious 
as  the  discussion  proceeds. 

A  ray  incident  to  the  front  surface  of  the  eye  which 
is  directed  through  the  first  nodal  point  emerges  into 
the  vitreous  directed  through  the  second  nodal  point 
and  parallel  to  the  incident  path  of  the  ray,  as  shown 
in  figure  5.  A  second  incident  ray  which  passes 
through  the  primary  focal  point  F  emerges  into  the 
vitreous  parallel  to  the  optic  axis.  The  emerging  ray 
is  also  directed  through  the  points  V  and  (".  This  is 
true  because  an  incident  ray  which  is  directed 
through  the  point  V  in  the  primary  principal  plane 
must  emerge  into  the  vitreous  directed  through  the 
point  I'  in  the  second  principal  plane  which  lies  on 
a  line  through  V  parallel  to  the  optic  axis.  This  is  a 
consequence  of  the  fact  that  the  principal  planes  are 
the  conjugate  planes  of  unit  magnification.  A  third 
incident  ray  parallel  to  the  optic  axis  emerges  into  the 
vitreous  directed  through  the  secondary  focal  point 
F'.  All  three  of  these  rays  which  emerge  into  the 
vitreous  converge  at  the  image  point  Q'.  It  is  obvious 
that  once  the  cardinal  points  and  planes  are  located 
we  can  predict  the  location  of  the  image  of  any  object 
point. 

The  cardinal  points  bear  fixed  relations  to  each 
other  so  that  once  the  principal  points  and  one  of 
the  focal  points  are  given,  the  locations  of  the  other 
points  can  be  immediately  determined  b>'  means  o 


FIG.   2.   Conjugate  foci  in  object  and  image  space. 


650 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  equation 

H'F'   =  ^  (F//)  =  FjV  =  -  (M'FO 
n  n 

where  n'  is  the  index  of  the  vitreous  and  n  the  index 
of  air. 


SURFACE 


FIG.  3.  Snell's  law  of  refraction. 


Refracting  Power  of  the  Eye 

Since  the  locations  of  principal  points  and  the  ratio 
n'/n  are  relatively  fixed  from  eye  to  eye  and  remain 
relatively  unchanged  as  a  given  eye  accommodates, 
the  specification  of  the  distance  H'F'  tells  us  prac- 
ticalh'  all  we  need  to  know  about  the  refracting 
mechanism  of  an  eye.  Instead  of  specifying  this  dis- 
tance, which  is  known  as  the  secondary  focal  length, 
it  is  more  usual  to  specify  the  refracting  power  F, 
but 

F  =  n'/H'F'. 

The  refracting  power  is  expressed  in  diopters. 

The  refracting  power  of  an  eye  is  dependent  upon 
the  curvature  of  the  refracting  surfaces  and  the  indices 
of  the  media.  In  all  cases  except  for  the  back  surface 


FIG.  4.  Cardinal  points  of  the  GuUstrand  schematic  eye.  In  this  eye  the  secondary  focus  F'  falls 
0.387  mm  behind  the  retina,  i.e.  the  layer  of  photosensitive  elements  that  respond  to  light.  The 
principal  points  //and //'are  located  1.348  mm  and  1.602  mm  from  the  front  surface  of  the  cornea, 
respectively.  FH  =  M'F'  =  17055  mm  and  H'F'  =  FJV  =  22.785  mm.  The  ratio  H'F'/FH  is 
equal  to  the  index  of  the  vitreous. 


FIG.  5.  Ray  tracing. 


0 

M                       ^>^                       H 

^-^N^N'             ^\f'                                  **' 

H'                 ^"~"'~~ „,,^^         \^ 

vl 

V                                                                                  ( 

3' 

THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF   THE    EYE  65 1 


of  the  cornea,  an  increase  in  curvature  increases  the 
refracting  power  of  the  eye. 

When  we  want  to  know  how  a  change  in  index 
affects  the  refracting  power  of  the  eye,  we  have  to 
approach  the  matter  from  a  different  direction.  If  we 
introduce  a  thin  layer  of  air  between  the  adjoining 
media,  this  will  create  two  surfaces  for  each  surface 
except  the  first,  and  each  medium  will  be  bounded 
on  both  sides  by  air.  Introducing  such  layers  of  air 
would  not  affect  the  direction  of  the  refracted  rays. 
Figure  6  represents  an  exploded  diagram  showing 
each  medium  bounded  on  both  sides  by  air.  These 
elements  form  plus  and  minus  lenses  except  in  the 
ca.se  of  the  lens  nucleus  where  a  biconvex  lens  is 
formed,  and  in  the  case  of  the  vitreous  where  we 
must  deal  with  a  single  refracting  surface.  This  analy- 
sis of  the  optical  system  makes  it  easy  to  visualize 
what  happens  when  the  index  of  a  given  medium 
changes.  An  increase  in  index  will  increase  or  de- 
crease the  total  power  of  the  eye  depending  upon 
whether  the  refracting  effect  of  the  particular  element 
is  plus  or  minus. 

Helmhollz  Schematic  Eye 

Hclmholtz  (yg,  p.  152)  made  use  of  a  somewhat 
more  simplified  schematic  eye  than  that  employed 
by  Gullstrand.  In  the  Helmholtz  schematic  eye  the 
cornea  represents  a  single  refracting  surface  at  which 
the  aqueous  adjoins  the  air.  Furthermore  the  lens  is 
treated  as  having  a  uniform  index  throughout  (see 

fig-  7)- 

The  value  which  Helmholtz  selected  for  the  radius 

of  the  front  surface  of  the  eye  approximates  the  average 

front  surface  of  the  cornea  in  the  adult  human  eye. 

The  displacement  of  the  lens  from  the  front  surface 

of  the  eye  also  approximates  the  distance  from  the 

front  surface  of  the  cornea  to  the  front  surface  of  the 

lens  as   measured   experimentally.   The   thickness  of 

the  lens  and  the  radii  of  curvature  also  appioximate 

the  actual  values.  The  value  of  1.338  given  to  the 


CORNEA 


CORNEA    I     CORTEX   |    CORTEX     | 

AQUEOUS        NUCLEUS       VITREOUS 

FIG.  6.  Exploded  diagram  of  the  Gullstrand  schematic  eye 
showing  the  various  elements  with  air  spaces  between  them. 


FIG.   7.  The  Helmholtz  schematic  eye 


index  of  the  aqueous  and  vitreous  for  sodium  light. 
(589  m|t)  approximates  the  true  value.  A  value 
of  1.455  ^35  been  selected  for  the  lens  because,  if 
the  lens  substance  is  assumed  to  have  a  uniform 
index  throughout,  it  gives  the  lens  approximately  the 
same  refracting  power  as  an  actual  lens  immersed  in 
vitreous.  The  indices  selected  by  Helmholtz  have 
been  adjusted  by  Laurance  (54)  from  1.338  to  1.333 
(or  J--Q  and  from  1.455  to  1.45  in  order  to  give  the 
eye  primary  and  secondary  focal  lengths  of  —  15  and 
20  mm,  respectively,  which  are  round  numbers. 

The  radii  of  curvature  of  the  refracting  surfaces, 
their  locations  and  the  indices  of  the  media  constitute 
the  optical  constants  of  the  eye  and  are  summarized 
for  the  Helmholtz  schematic  eye  in  table  i.  All  of 
these  values  refer  to  sodium  light  (589  mix). 

Reduced  Eye 

Helmholtz's  schematic  eye  can  be  simplified  still 
further,  as  has  been  done  by  Laurance  (54),  by 
using  a  single  refracting  surface  as  shown  in  figure  8. 
The  interior  of  this  eye  is  filled  with  a  medium  which 
has  the  same  index  throughout  and  is  equivalent  to 
that  of  the  vitreous  of  the  Helmholtz  schematic  eye, 
namely  1.333.  The  surface  at  which  this  medium 
makes  contact  with  the  air  in  front  of  the  eye  is  the 
only  refracting  surface.  The  curvature  of  this  surface 
has  been  arbitrarily  increased  to  compensate  for 
the  absence  of  the  lens  so  that  the  eye  has  the  same 
refracting  power  as  the  Helmholtz  schematic  eye. 

In  the  reduced  eye  the  two  principal  planes  coin- 
cide and  are  tangent  to  the  front  surface  of  the  eye. 
The  two  nodal  points  coincide  at  the  center  of  curva- 
ture of  the  front  surface.  The  focal  lengths  are  the 
same  as  in  the  Helmholtz  schematic  eye,  and  for 
most  purposes  the  reduced  eye  is  equivalent  to  the 
Helmholtz  eye.  It  is  very  useful  for  visualizing  certain 


652 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOG\' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


TABLE  I .  Optical  Constants  of  the  Helmholtz  Schematic  Eye 

Distance  from  cornea  to  front  of  lens 

I'hickness  of  lens 

Radii  of  curvature: 

Cornea 

Front  surface  of  lens  .  . 

Back  surface  of  lens 

Indices  of  refraction  (sodium  light) 

Aqueous i  .333 

Lens 1 .45 

Vitreous    1 .333 


■i-*^ 

mm 

:3f^ 

mm 

8 

mm 

0 

mm 

6 

mm 

FIG.  8.  The  reduced  eye  devised  by  Laurance. 


aspects  of  image  formation  as  will   become  ob\ious 
later. 


Role  of  Pupil  in  Image  Formation 

The  pupil  is  an  aperture  in  the  iris  which  lies  in 
contact  with  the  front  surface  of  the  lens.  The  pupil 
varies  in  size  because  the  muscles  in  the  iris  can  make 
it  either  contract  or  dilate.  The  pupil  is  important  in 
the  formation  of  an  image  on  the  retina  because  by 
changing  its  size  it  can  aflfect  the  illuminance  and  the 
blur  of  the  image.  It  does  this  by  limiting  the  size 
of  the  ray  bundle  which  enters  the  eye  from  each 
object  point.  In  terms  of  geometrical  optics  this 
means  that  it  serves  as  the  aperture  stop  of  the 
system. 

In  order  to  understand  the  role  of  the  pupil  in 
image  formation,  it  simplifies  matters  to  make  use  of 
the  imaginary  entrance-  and  exit-pupils  of  the  eye. 
The  entrance-pupil  is  conjugate  to  the  real  pupil 
with  respect  to  refraction  at  the  cornea,  and  the  exit- 
pupil  is  conjugate  to  the  entrance-pupil  with  respect 
to  the  complete  refracting  mechanism  of  the  eye.  The 
entrance-pupil  is  larger  than  the  real  pupil  and  lies 
slightly  in  front  of  it.  The  exit-pupil  lies  behind  the 
real  pupil  and  is  not  quite  as  lai-ge  as  the  entrance- 


pupil.  The  entrance-pupil  is  the  pupil  which  we  see 
when  we  look  at  another  person's  e\e.  This  is  the 
pupil  on  which  direct  measurements  can  be  made  in 
\isual  experiments.  Its  position  and  diameter  can  be 
directly  determined.  The  positions  and  sizes  of  the 
real  and  of  the  cxit-piipil  have  to  be  calculated. 

Chief  Rays 

Another  concept  which  is  needed  in  explaining  the 
role  of  the  pupil  is  that  of  a  chief  ray.  The  chief  rav 
of  a  bundle  of  rays  entering  the  pupil  of  the  eye  from 
a  given  object  point  is  the  one  which  is  directed 
through  the  center  (0)  of  the  entrance-pupil  and 
which,  after  refraction  at  the  cornea,  passes  through 
the  center  of  the  real  pupil.  After  emerging  into  the 
vitreous,  it  is  directed  through  the  center  (0')  of  the 
exit-pupil,  as  shown  in  figure  9. 

Blur  Circles  in  Eye  Free  from  Astigmatism 

The  major  role  of  the  pupil  is  to  limit  the  size  of 
the  blur  circles  and  ellipses  formed  on  the  retina  when 
an  eye  is  out  of  focus.  The  schematic  and  reduced  eyes 
referred  to  above  are  all  free  from  astigmatism  for 
object  points  close  to  the  optic  axis  because  the  re- 
fracting surfaces  are  assumed  to  be  spherical  and  also 
centered  on  the  optic  axis.  In  this  eye  free  from 
astigmatism,  the  bundle  of  rays  from  a  given  object 
point  emerges  into  the  vitreous  as  a  cone  or  pencil  of 
rays  with  the  exit  pupil  forming  the  base  and  with  the 
rays  coming  to  a  focus  at  the  apex  as  shown  in  figure 
10.  This  point  is  called  the  optical  image  and  may 
lie  on,  in  front  of,  or  behind  the  retina.  If  the  retina 
intercepts  the  bundle  at  the  optical  image  so  that  the 
optical  image  falls  on  the  retina,  the  retinal  image  in 
terms  of  geometrical  theory  is  a  point  image;  but  if 
the  optical  image  ()'  falls  in  front  of  or  behind  the 
retina,  the  retinal  image  at  Q.'  is  an  out-of -focus  blur 
circle.  As  is  obvious  from  figure  10,  the  size  of  the 
blur  circle  is  determined  by  the  size  of  the  exit-pupil. 

Astigmatism 

The  bundle  of  rays  from  a  point  source  does  not 
always  come  to  a  focus  at  a  point.  The  most  common 
deviation  from  this  ideal  is  called  astigmatism.  Figure 
I  I  illustrates  an  astigmatic  bundle  of  rays  emerging 
from  the  exit-pupil.  The  planes  which  intersect  at  the 
chief  ray  constitute  the  meridians  of  the  bundle.  The 
vertical  and  horizontal  meridians  are  the  principal 
meridians  because  the  ravs  come  to  a  focus  in  these 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE  653 

Fig.  10 


Fic.  9.   Chief  ray. 


FIG.    10.   Out-of-focus  blur  circle. 


meridians.  The  rays  in  the  vertical  meridian  focus  at 
Q,'  and  the  rays  in  the  horizontal  meridian  at  QJ'. 
Cross  sections  of  the  bundle  at  various  distances 
from  the  exit-pupil  are  also  shown  in  the  figure.  The 
cross  section  is  in  general  elliptical  but  at  Q,'  it  be- 
comes a  horizontal  line  and  at  QJ'  a  vertical  line.  At 
one  point  in  between  it  becomes  a  circle;  and  when 
this  part  of  the  bundle  is  intercepted  by  the  retina, 
the  effect  is  the  same  as  throwing  out  of  focus  an 
eye  which  is  free  from  astigmatism. 

There  are  two  principal  causes  of  astigmatism. 
On  the  one  hand  the  chief  ray  of  the  bundle  may  be 
oblique  to  one  or  more  of  the  refracting  surfaces.  On 
the  other  hand  one  or  more  of  the  surfaces  may  be 
toroidal,  i.e.  a  given  surface  may  be  shaped  like  the 
side  of  a  barrel  which  is  more  curved  in  the  direction 
around  the  barrel  than  up  and  down.  It  is  obvious 
that  in  a  multisurface  system  like  the  eye,  there  may 
be  many  comijinations  of  toroidal  and  tilted  surfaces 
and  it  becomes  impractical  to  try  to  analyze  all  these 
various  combinations. 

In  practice  the  resultant  astigmatism  is  measured 
and  treated  without  analyzing  the  contributions 
made  by  the  separate  surfaces.  However,  the  toroidic- 
ity  of  the  cornea  may  be  independently  measured  and 
gross  observations  may  be  made  of  the  tilt  of  the  re- 
fracting surfaces. 

Lines  of  Sight 

Object  points  which  lie  on  the  incident  path  of  a 
chief  ray  produce  concentric  blur  circles  or  ellipses 
on  the  retina,  and  because  of  this  the  incident  path  of 
a  chief  ray  is  also  called  a  line  of  sight. 

Primary  Line  of  Sig/it  and  Foveal  Chief  Ray 

Of  all  of  the  lines  of  sight  (or  chief  rays)  which 
converge  at  the  center  of  the  entrance-pupil,  there  is 


CHIEF 
RAY 


CIRCLE   OF 
LEAST   CONFUSION 


FIG.  1 1.  .-Xstigmatic  blur  ellipses. 


one  which  is  all-important  in  the  use  of  the  eyes.  When 
a  person  is  told  to  fixate  a  given  point  with  one  of  his 
eyes  while  the  other  is  covered,  he  points  that  eye 
at  the  object.  The  pointing  is  not  steady  because  the 
eye  is  subject  to  a  fine  tremor  and  also  weaves  back 
and  forth  and  makes  occasional  jerks  awa\'  from  the 
object,  but  we  can  nevertheless  think  of  the  average 
fixating  position  of  the  normal  eye  as  one  in  which  the 
retinal  image  is  centered  on  a  given  part  of  the  retina 
which  falls  somewhere  near  the  center  of  the  fo\'ea. 
The  chief  ray  which  penetrates  this  point  is  the  fo\eal 
chief  ray  and  the  incident  path  of  this  ra\  is  the  pri- 
mary line  of  sight. 

Pupillary  Axis  am!  Angle  X 

It  is  customary  to  specify  the  location  of  the  fo\enl 
line  of  sight  in  terms  of  its  relation  to  the  pupillary 
axis  which  can  be  easily  located  by  ol)jective  methods. 
The  pupillary  axis  is  a  line  normal  to  the  front  surface 
of  the  cornea  and  directed  through  the  center  of  the 
entrance-pupil.  It  forms  an  angle  X  with  the  primary 
line  of  sight  which  also  passes  through  the  center  of 


654 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  12.  The  angle  X. 

the  entrance-pupil.  The  pupillary  axis  is  usually 
about  5  degrees  temporalward  from  the  primary  line 
of  sight,  as  appears  in  figure  12. 

Size  of  Retinal  Image 

The  chief  ray  concept  is  also  useful  in  dealing  with 
the  size  of  the  retinal  image  of  an  external  object.  The 
angle  7  subtended  by  two  object  points  at  the  center 
of  the  entrance-pupil  0  is  called  the  visual  angle;  it 
is  the  angle  between  the  two  lines  of  sight  (see  fig.  9). 

The  term  "size  of  the  retinal  image'  refers  to  the 
linear  distance  between  the  retinal  images  M"  and 
Q,"  of  the  two  object  points  M  and  Q,.  When  the  eye 
is  out  of  focus,  the  retinal  images  are  blur  circles  and 
the  size  M"Q_"  represents  the  distance  between  the 
centers  of  the  two  blur  circles  formed  on  the  retina. 
If  the  angle  y'  subtended  by  the  centers  of  the  two  blur 
circles  at  the  center  of  the  exit-pupil  is  expressed  in 
radians, 

linear  separation  of  the 

centers  of  two  blur  circles 
y'  =  . 

distance  from  the  exit  pupil  to  the  retina 

The  ratio  of  7'  to  7  is  one  of  the  more  important 
constants  of  the  eye.  In  the  case  of  the  Helmholtz 
schematic  eye 

7'/7  =  0.81. 

With  this  ratio  and  the  distance  from  the  exit-pupil 
to  the  retina  specified,  one  can  compute  the  linear 
separation  of  two  blur  circles  on  the  retina  for  a  given 
value  of  7. 

The  expression  'size  of  the  retinal  image'  is  often 
misinterpreted  to  mean  the  size  of  the  blur  circle 
formed  by  a  single  object  point,  but  this  is  something 
which  is  quite  different  from  the  linear  separation 
between  the  centers  of  two  blur  circles. 


REFRACTION  AND  ACCOMMODATION  OF  THE  EYE 

The  eye  has  an  adjustable  focusing  mechanism. 
The  first  section  of  this  chapter  has  explained  how  the 
eye  forms  an  image  of  an  object  when  the  focusing 
mechanism  is  fixed.  This  section  describes  how  the 
changes  in  focus  may  be  described  and  specified. 

Refraction  of  the  Eye 

If  a  given  point  on  the  primary  line  of  sight  pro- 
duces a  bundle  of  rays  which  comes  to  a  point  focus 
on  the  retina,  the  eye  is  said  to  be  focused  for  this 
point.  Another  way  of  stating  this  is  to  say  that  the 
object  point  producing  the  bundle  is  conjugate  to 
the  retina. 

The  spectacle  point  5 (14  mm  in  front  of  the  cornea) 
is  used  as  the  reference  point  for  specifying  the  loca- 
tion of  the  point  R  which  is  conjugate  to  the  retina, 
as  represented  in  figure  13.4.  Stating  the  distance 
from  R  to  S  adequately  describes  the  refractive  state 
of  the  eye,  but  it  is  customary  to  use  the  reciprocal  of 
this  distance  and  call  it  the  refraction  of  the  eye.  It 
is  measured  in  diopters  when  the  distance  RS  is  given 
in  meters. 

When  the  eye  is  astigmatic,  it  is  necessary  to 
specify  separately  the  refraction  in  the  two  principal 
meridians.  To  visualize  this  problem  it  is  better  to 
start  with  a  point  on  the  retina  penetrated  by  the 
foveal  chief  ray  and  trace  a  bundle  of  rays  back  out 
of  the  eye  through  the  entrance-pupil.  The  line  of 
sight  represents  the  chief  ray  of  this  bundle,  and  the 
principal  meridians  are  the  planes  which  intersect  at 
right  angles  at  the  line  of  sight. 

The  o  to  180  degree  meridian  which  is  the  reference 
meridian  for  the  location  of  the  principal  meridians 
lies  in  the  plane  of  regard  which  is  defined  by  the 
centers  of  the  two  entrance-pupils  and  the  point  of 
convergence  of  the  two  primary  lines  of  sight.  In  figure 
1 3^  the  line  of  sight  is  perpendicular  to  the  paper  and 
penetrates  the  front  of  the  eye  at  A.  The  angles  0i  and 


-I4fnm 
A.  SPECTACLE      POINT 


FIG.    13.    Reference    points    and    planes    for    specifying    the 
refraction  of  an  eye. 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE 


655 


02  represent  the  counterclockwise  angular  displace- 
ment of  the  principal  meridians  from  the  o  to  180 
degree  meridian. 

Accommodation 

The  refracting  mechanism  of  the  eye  possesses  the 
ability  of  accommodating  itself  for  different  distances; 
that  is  to  say,  the  eye  can  focus  on  one  object  at  a 
given  moment  and  on  an  object  at  a  diflferent  distance 
a  moment  later.  This  represents  a  change  in  the  re- 
fractive state  of  the  eye  which  is  brought  about  by  a 
change  in  form  of  the  lens  and  a  slight  movement  for- 
ward. The  major  part  of  this  efTect  is  mediated  by  the 
change  in  curvature  of  the  front  surface  of  the  lens, 
and  it  greatly  simplifies  our  concept  of  how  the  eye 
works  if  we  assume  that  this  is  the  only  variable.  If 
as  in  the  schematic  eye  the  pupil  is  centered  upon  the 
optical  axis  of  the  lens,  chief  rays  through  the  center 
of  the  pupil  cross  the  axis  at  the  front  surface  of  the 
lens  and  the  refraction  of  such  rays  is  not  affected  by 
a  change  in  curvature.  There  is  therefore  no  change 
in  the  ratio  of  7'  to  7. 

Static  Refraction  of  the  E\e 

When  accommodation  is  relaxed,  the  point  R  for 
which  the  eye  is  accommodated  in  a  given  meridian 
is  known  as  the  far  point  Qmncliim  remotum")  for  that 
meridian.  The  reciprocal  Qi/RS')  of  the  distance  from 
the  far  point  (R)  to  the  spectacle  point  (5)  is  defined 
as  the  static  refraction  of  the  eye  and  is  measured  in 
diopters.  The  spectacle  point  corresponds  to  the  back 
surface  of  a  spectacle  lens  and  lies  14  mm  in  front  of 
the  cornea.  For  all  practical  purposes  it  coincides 
with  the  primary  focal  point. 

Emmetropia  is  the  condition  in  which  the  far 
point  lies  at  infinity  and  in  which  the  static  refraction 
equals  zero.  Ametropia  is  the  condition  in  which  the 
far  point  does  not  lie  at  infinity  but  at  some  finite  dis- 
tance either  in  front  of  or  behind  the  spectacle  point. 
Myopia  is  the  positive  t)  pe  of  ametropia  in  which  the 
far  point  lies  at  some  finite  distance  in  front  of  the 
spectacle  point.  Hyperopia  is  the  negative  type  of 
ametropia  in  which  the  far  point  lies  behind  the 
spectacle  point. 

When  an  eye  is  equally  ametropic  in  all  meridians, 
it  is  said  to  have  a  spherical  error  of  refraction  which 
may  be  either  hyperopic  or  myopic.  When  the  static 
refraction  differs  in  the  various  meridians,  the  eye  is 
said  to  have  an  astigmatic  error  of  refraction. 


Correction  for  Ametropia 

That  lens  which  will  permit  an  ametrope  to  see 
lines  clearly  in  all  meridians  at  6  m  with  relaxed  ac- 
commodation is  called  the  distance  correction.  The 
myope  needs  a  minus  lens  and  the  hyperope  a  plus 
lens,  as  shown  in  figure  14.  In  each  case  the  refracting 
power  of  the  lens  is  the  same  in  all  meridians  and  is 
equal  numerically  to  the  static  refraction  but  opposite 
in  sign.  Myopia  for  example  is  a  positive  ametropia 
which  is  neutralized  with  a  minus  lens. 

A  person  with  astigmatism  requires  a  lens  which 
varies  in  power  from  meridian  to  meridian.  Such  a 
lens  has  a  toric  or  cylindrical  surface  on  one  side  and 
a  plane  or  spherical  surface  on  the  other  and  is 
equivalent  to  a  combination  of  a  spherical  lens  with 
a  cylindrical  lens.  The  cylindrical  lens  compensates 
the  astigmatic  component  of  the  refractive  error,  and 
the  spherical  lens  compensates  the  residual  spherical 
component. 

In  designing  a  lens  for  a  given  person  many  com- 
binations of  surfaces  may  be  used  on  the  two  sides 
to  provide  the  correction  for  the  ametropia,  and 
other  factors  have  to  be  considered  in  selecting  the 
particular  curves  to  be  used.  The  thickness  and  index 
can  also  be  varied  although  the  glass  normally  used 
has  an  index  of  1.523  for  sodium  light.  The  lens  may 
be  designed  to  compensate  for  its  own  aberrations,  to 
provide  a  specified  amount  of  angular  magnification 
in  addition  to  refracting  power  and  to  minimize  re- 
flections, and  some  consideration  is  always  given  to 
breakage  and  weight  on  the  face.  Plastic  lenses  are 
sometimes  used  instead  of  glass  lenses. 

The  ordinary  ophthalmic  lens  is  mounted  with  its 
back  surface  at  or  near  the  spectacle  point  14  mm  from 
the  cornea  and  with  its  optic  axis  passing  through 
the  center  of  rotation  of  the  eye. 

A  corneal  contact  lens  is  worn  in  contact  with  the 
cornea,  while  the  scleral  type  contact  lens  contacts 


FIG.    14.    Spectacle   lenses   for    the    correction    of  spherical 
ametropia. 


656 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  sclera  around  the  cornea  with  a  thin  fluid  layer 
between  the  contact  lens  and  the  cornea. 


Specification  of  Amount  of  AccommodatKin  m  Play 

Accommodation  is  measured  in  diopters  and  repre- 
sents the  reciprocal  of  the  distance  from  the  conjugate 
focus  of  the  retina  to  the  spectacle  point  when  the 
distance  correction  is  worn.  Thus  when  the  distance 
correction  is  worn  the  accommodation  must  be  re- 
duced to  its  zero  level  in  order  to  see  a  distant  object 
clearly. 

The  ma.ximal  amount  of  accommodation  that  can 
be  elicited  is  called  the  amplitude  of  accommodation. 
The  nearest  point  for  which  the  eye  can  accommodate 
is  called  the  near  point  of  accommodation  (^punctum 
proxirnurtt).  This  varies  with  age  as  shown  in  figure  15. 
At  the  age  of  about  40,  the  near  point  recedes  rapidly 
beyond  the  ordinary  working  distance  of  33  to  40  cm. 
\'arious  sets  of  data  relating  amplitude  to  age  have 
been  analyzed  by  Marg  et  al.  (63).  It  is  necessary  to 
compensate  for  this  loss  of  accommodation  with  a 
plus  lens  added  to  the  distance  correction.  The  added 
plus  lens  power  at  the  near  point  is  usuallv  pro\ided 
in  the  form  of  a  bifocal  which  has  enough  plus  lens 
power  added  in  the  segment  to  permit  the  eye  to  see 
clearly  at  a  given  working  distance  with  one  half  of 
the  accommodation  held  in  reserve. 

Ap/iafcia 

When  the  crystalline  lens  is  missing,  the  eye  is 
said  to  be  aphakic.  The  lens  may  be  surgically  re- 
moved, it  may  be  congenitally  absent,  or  it  may  be 
dislocated  so  that  it  fails  to  cover  the  pupillary  aper- 
ture. Clear  vision  can  still  be  obtained  by  means  of  a 
lens  mounted  in  front  of  it,  but  the  eye  no  longer  has 
any  power  of  accommodation.  The  range  of  clear 
vision  through  any  given  lens  is  strictly  a  function  of 
the  depth  of  the  focus  of  the  eye. 


FIG.  15.  Regression  of  the 
near  point  of  accommodation 
with  age.  Based  on  Donder's 
data  (20).  It  is  assumed  that  the 
distance  correction  is  worn. 


20       30      40 

iGE  (YEARS) 


OCUL.AR    ME.ASUREMENTS 

In  the  first  section  of  this  chapter  reference  was 
made  to  the  indices  of  the  media  and  the  curvature 
of  the  refracting  surfaces  and  their  distances  in  front 
ot  the  retina.  In  the  second  section,  reference  was 
made  to  the  distance  from  the  conjugate  focus  of  the 
retina  to  the  spectacle  point  and  its  use  in  specifying 
refraction  and  accommodation.  This  section  explains 
how  such  quantities  are  actually  measured. 

Indices  of  Media 

Each  medium  of  the  eye  has  been  assumed  to  be 
uniform  in  the  schematic  eye;  but  the  cornea  and  the 
lens  have  a  definite  microstructure  and  can  be 
treated  as  made  up  of  layers  of  different  indices. 

The  cornea  has  five  different  layers,  the  epithelium, 
Bowman's  membrane,  the  stroma,  Descemet's  mem- 
brane and  the  endothelium.  The  index  for  the 
epithelium  has  been  found  to  be  1.416  and  that  for 
the  rest  of  the  cornea,  1.372  (22,  p.  728). 

The  index  of  the  lens  substance  varies  from  1.387 
at  the  cortex  to  1.406  at  the  center  (22,  p.  736).  The 
variation  in  index  of  the  lens  from  the  center  to  the 
cortex  affects  its  performance  as  an  image-forming 
device.  The  problem  can  be  formulated  by  visualizing 
a  series  of  isoindical  surfaces  from  the  cortex  to  the 
center.  In  a  meridian  section  these  surfaces  probably 
correspond  to  the  course  of  the  fibers  as  they  arch 
around   the  nucleus  from  the  axis  back  to  the  axis 

(79.  P-  339)- 

The  index  of  the  aqueous  is  1.336  and  that  of  the 

vitreous  is  the  same  (22,  p.  734). 

The  index  measurements  reported  above  ha\e  all 

been  made  with  an  Abbe  refractometer  which  makes 

use  of  the  principle  of  total  reflection. 

Purkinje  Images 

The  Purkinje  images  are  important  because  they 
provide  us  with  many  objective  methods  of  studying 
the  configuration,  tilt  and  location  of  the  refracting 
surfaces.  The  refracting  surfaces  of  the  eye  can  form 
images  both  by  reflection  and  refraction.  Images  in- 
volving single  reflection  at  the  front  and  iiack  surfaces 
of  the  cornea  and  at  the  front  and  iiack  surfaces  of  the 
lens  are  known  as  the  first,  second,  third  and  fourth 
Purkinje  images  (86,  p.  48).  These  images  may  be 
seen  bv  an  observer  located  in  front  of  the  eye. 


THE    1M.\C;E-F()R.ML\G    mechanism    OF    THE    EYE 


657 


Optic  Axis  0/  the  Eye 

It  has  been  assumed  in  connection  with  the  sche- 
matic eye  that  the  refracting  surfaces  are  centered  on 
a  common  axis,  but  the  extent  to  which  this  is  true 
in  the  case  of  an  actual  eye  can  be  tested  by  observing 
the  Purkinje  images  of  a  point  of  light  held  close  to 
the  observer's  eye.  The  subject  is  then  made  to  follow 
a  fixation  target  which  is  moved  about  until  the 
Purkinje  images  line  up  or  at  least  until  the  third  and 
fourth  ones  line  up.  This  puts  the  observer's  eye  on 
the  path  of  a  ray  of  light  which  passes  through  the 
lens  normal  to  both  surfaces,  and  the  extent  to  which 
the  center  of  the  pupil  and  the  center  of  curvature  of 
the  front  surface  of  the  cornea  are  displaced  from  the 
optic  axis  of  the  lens  can  then  be  directly  observed. 

In  general  the  optic  axis  (in  so  far  a  such  an  axis 
exists)  coincides  with  the  pupillary  axis,  so  that  the 
optic  axis  deviates  temporalward  from  the  primary 
line  of  sight  about  5  degrees  and  also  about  2  degrees 
downward  (86,  p.  77).  For  the  purpose  of  specifying 
this  angle,  it  may  be  assumed  that  the  two  lines  inter- 
sect at  the  center  of  the  entrance-pupil. 

The  optic  axis  does  not  necessarily  coincide  with 
the  anatomical  axis  of  the  eye  which  may  be  defined 
as  the  line  connecting  the  geometrical  center  of  the 
cornea  (front  pole)  with  the  geometrical  center  of 
the  sclera  (back  pole).  However,  the  optic  axis 
and  the  anatomical  axis  do  approximately  coincide 
in  the  average  eye.  Theoretically  the  anatomical 
axis  of  the  eye  should  coincide  with  the  line 
normal  to  the  cornea  at  its  geometrical  center. 
This  is  called  the  geometrical  axis  of  the  cornea. 

Coiifigmalion  of  Front  Surface  of  Cornea 

The  central  portion  of  the  cornea  is  usually  spherical 
or  toroidal.  With  a  keratometer  one  can  determine 
whether  the  cornea  is  spherical  or  toroidal  and,  if  it 
is  toroidal,  the  principal  meridians  can  also  be  deter- 
mined. Furthermore,  one  can  measure  the  radius  of 
curvature  in  each  of  the  principal  meridians  of  a 
toroidal  cornea  and  in  any  meridian  of  a  spherical 
cornea. 

The  central  portion  of  the  typical  cornea  which 
may  be  regarded  as  spherical  or  toroidal  covers  a 
region  about  4  mm  in  diameter  and  outside  of  this 
area  the  curvature  gradually  decreases  as  the  limbus 
is  approached.  The  center  of  the  optical  portion  does 
not  necessarily  fall  at  the  center  of  the  cornea  (79, 
p.  311 ;  86,  p.  68). 

The  exact  form  of  the  peripheral  portion  of  the 
cornea  can  be  investigated  in  a  number  of  ways.  One 


can  view  the  profile  of  the  cornea  with  a  microscope 
or  photograph  the  profile.  One  can  examine  it  point 
by  point  with  an  ordinary  keratometer  by  using  a 
variable  point  of  fixation.  It  can  also  be  viewed  with 
a  keratoscope  or  photographed  with  a  photokerato- 
scope.  In  this  technique  a  reflected  image  of  a  series 
of  concentric  circles  is  used.  In  one  of  the  later  models 
(50)  the  concentric  rings  are  arranged  on  a  spherical 
surface  concentric  with  the  eye  so  that  the  reflected 
images  cover  the  entire  cornea.  Another  method  is 
that  of  sprinkling  powder  on  the  cornea  and  then 
taking  a  stereophotograph  which  can  later  be  ana- 
lyzed like  an  aerial  map.  One  can  also  take  a  mold 
of  the  cornea  as  in  fitting  contact  lenses  and  then 
studv  the  configuration  of  the  mold. 


Measurement  of  Internal  Refracting  Surfaces 

One  can  measure  the  position  of  the  margin  of  the 
iris  with  respect  to  the  cornea  and  assume  that  this 
lies  in  contact  with  the  front  surface  of  the  lens  (79, 
p.  19,  334).  The  ophthalmophakometer  (86,  p.  80) 
and  the  Blix  ophthalmometer  (79,  p.  326)  make  use 
of  specular  reflections  at  the  surfaces  to  locate  the 
positions  of  the  vertices  and  centers  of  curvature  of  the 
surfaces.  It  is  also  possible  to  photograph  the  Purkinje 
images  (i,  2,  6,  45,  49,  94)  and  to  calculate  the  radii 
of  curvature  of  the  reflecting  surfaces  from  this  kind 
of  data. 

Fincham  (26,  p.  38)  used  diffusely  reflected  light 
to  produce  an  optical  section  of  the  refracting  surfaces, 
and  lay  viewing  with  a  microscope  having  a  cali- 
brated fore  and  aft  movement  he  was  able  to  measure 
directly  the  apparent  separations  of  the  surfaces. 
A  similar  arrangement  can  be  used  with  a  camera 
replacing  the  microscope  (26,  p.  44).  A  projective 
transformation  of  the  photographic  image  gives  a 
cross  section  of  the  eye.  This  inethod  has  the  ad- 
vantage of  showing  not  only  the  configurations  of 
the  surfaces  but  also  gives  the  internal  structure  of 
the  lens. 

The  measureiTients  made  on  an  internal  surface 
appl\-  to  the  apparent  surface  viewed  through  the 
refracting  surfaces  lying  in  front  of  the  surface  in 
question.  The  concept  of  a  thick  mirror  is  helpful  in 
this  connection.  The  center  of  curvature  of  the 
apparent  surface  is  the  image  of  the  actual  center  of 
curvature  formed  by  the  refracting  surfaces  lying 
in  front,  and  the  vertex  of  the  apparent  surface  is 
also  the  image  of  the  vertex  of  the  actual  surface. 


6^8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


X-ray  Measurement  of  Axial  Length  of  the  Eye 

A  sheet  of  X-rays  is  produced  by  passing  X-rays 
through  two  parallel  slits,  and  the  eye  is  held  so  that 
this  sheet  of  rays  traverses  the  eye  perpendicular  to 
the  direction  of  regard,  as  shown  in  figure  i6.  Its 
intersection  with  the  retina  is  a  circle  and,  since 
X-rays  so  applied  stimulate  the  retina,  the  subject 
sees  a  circle  of  light.  The  size  of  the  circle  can  be  made 
smaller  and  smaller  by  moving  the  sheet  of  rays 
toward  the  back  of  the  eye,  keeping  it  always  per- 
pendicular to  the  direction  of  regard.  It  reduces  to  a 
point  as  it  becomes  tangent  to  the  retina.  The  distance 
of  the  cornea  from  this  plane  can  then  be  measured  by 
sighting  on  the  profile  of  the  cornea  (23).  (Care 
must  be  taken  to  keep  the  X-rays  from  passing  through 
the  crystalline  lens.) 

X-ray  Determination  oj  Location  of  Second  .Nodal  Point 

Two  sheets  of  X-rays  are  produced  by  passing 
X-rays  from  a  source  through  one  slit  and  these  then 
are  passed  again  through  two  slits  which  are  parallel 
to  the  first  slit  as  shown  in  figure  17.  These  two  sheets 
of  rays  are  allowed  to  traverse  the  back  of  the  eye 
which  is  pointed  with  its  optic  axis  -in  a  direction 
parallel  to  the  slits.  Two  distant  object  points  are 
adjusted  so  that  their  images  fall  on  the  two  lines 
on  the  retina  stimulated  by  the  X-rays.  Knowing  the 
linear  separation  of  the  two  X-ray  images  and 
knowing  the  angular  separation  of  the  two  optical 
images  at  the  second  nodal  point,  one  can  compute 
the  distance  from  the  second  nodal  point  to  the  retina. 


Locating  Conjugate  Focus  of  Retina 

It  is  assumed  in  this  section  that  the  astigmatism 
of  the  eye,  if  any  be  present,  has  been  corrected  and 
that  the  experimenter  is  interested  only  in  locating 
the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina  for  the  purpose  of 
determining  the  refractive  state  of  the  eye  or  the 
amount  of  accommodation  in  play.  In  order  to 
simplify  our  problem  let  us  consider  absolute  pres- 
byopia in  which  the  eye  has  a  fixed  focus.  The  same 
thing  occurs  in  an  aphakic  eye  or  in  an  eye  which  has 
been  temporarily  paralyzed  with  a  drug.  In  this 
case  a  target  is  needed  to  control  fixation  and  then 
some  subjective  or  objectiv'e  means  must  be  provided 
for  locating  the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina. 

Let  us  consider  the  subjective  methods  first.  A 
target  consisting  of  a  point,  a  line  or  a  row  of  small 
letters  can  be  used  for  controlling  fixation  and  lo- 
cating the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina.  The  distance 
can  be  varied  or  lenses  may  be  placed  in  front  of  the 
eyes.  The  natural  pupil  may  be  used  or,  if  this  has 
been  dilated,  a  diaphragm  with  a  pupil  of  normal 
size  can  be  placed  before  the  eye.  The  target  must 
be  fine  enough  so  that  the  subject  can  tell  when 
the  focus  is  sharpest. 

The  Scheiner  principle  substitutes  for  the  natural 
pupil  two  small  holes  or  two  parallel  slits.  If  a  single 
target  is  used  the  retinal  image  doubles  when  it  is 
out  of  focus  (fig.  18).  The  doubling  is  easier  to  detect 
when  monochromatic  light  is  used  than  when  white 
light  is  used;  still  better  the  two  beams  can  be  trans- 
mitted through  filters  of  different  color.  If  the  upper 
and  lo\ver  halves  of  a  vertical  slit  are  seen  through 
different  parts  of  the  pupil  displaced  laterally  from 


Fig  16 


Fig^l7  oPTK 


FIG.    16.  X-ray  measurement  o(  the  axial  length  of  the  eye.  fig.    17.   X-ray  method  of  locating  the  second  nodal  point. 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EVE 


^59 


each  other,  the  two  halves  of  the  slit  are  seen  out  of 
vertical  alignment  when  the  eye  is  out  of  focus 
(fig.  19).  Monochromatic  light  is  used  in  this  case. 
Since  the  Scheiner  technique  makes  use  of  only  two 
small  parts  of  the  pupil,  it  may  yield  a  different 
measure  of  accommodation  than  a  measurement 
based  upon  the  full  pupil.  This  is  a  function  of  the 
spherical  aberration  of  the  eye.  Consequently  the 
Scheiner  principle  is  primarily  useful  in  measuring 
changes  in  accommodation. 

The  threshold  principle  involves  having  the  target 
(usually  a  line)  disappear  when  it  goes  out  of  focus. 
This  test  has  the  advantage  that  it  can  be  used  with  a 
normal  pupil. 

The  retinoscope  (skiascope)  is  an  objective  device 
for  determining  the  refraction  of  the  eye.  A  small 
mirror  throws  a  beam  of  light  on  the  eye  from  a 
small  source.  The  examiner  looks  through  a  small 
hole  in  the  mirror  and  observes  the  light  reflected 
from  the  retina  back  out  of  the  eye.  This  makes  the 
pupil  appear  bright,  and  moving  the  mirror  modifies 
the  distribution  of  the  light  in  the  pupil  so  that  the 
examiner  can  tell  when  the  eye  is  out  of  focus  for  the 
hole  in  the  mirror.  When  the  eye  has  a  fixed  focus, 
the  subject  can  control  his  fixation  by  fixating  the 
mirror  image  of  the  light  source. 

The  coincidence  optometer  is  another  objective 
device  which  is  a  very  valuable  means  for  the  ob- 
jective determination  of  the  refractive  state.  In  this 
device  an  image  is  formed  on  the  pigment  epithelium 
in  contact  with  the  retina,  and  the  light  difl'uscly 
reflected   from    this  surface    forms   an   image   in   the 


plane  conjugate  to  the  retina.  This  image  is  viewed 
through  an  eye  piece.  The  target  and  the  focal  plane 
of  the  eye  piece  are  kept  at  the  same  distance  from 
the  subject's  eye,  but  this  distance  can  be  varied  to 
locate  the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina.  The  operator 
may  use  blur  as  a  criterion  for  the  proper  setting,  or  as 
in  Fincham's  instrument  (24,  27)  a  modification  of 
Scheiner's  principle  may  be  used.  Fincham  (un- 
published observations)  has  also  made  use  of  a 
photoelectric  cell  with  a  feed-back  which  auto- 
matically focuses  the  instrument. 

Campbell  (15)  has  designed  an  optometer  based 
on  the  use  of  a  photocell  and  Scheiner's  principle 
which  automatically  records  changes  in  accom- 
modation. 

The  indirect  ophthalmoscope  may  be  used  like  a 
coincidence  optometer  except  that  the  blood  vessels 
and  demarcations  on  the  retina  are  used  instead  of 
an  image  of  an  external  target  focused  on  the  retina. 
In  this  way  only  the  focus  of  the  emerging  beam  is 
involved  in  the  measurement. 

With  the  direct  ophthalmoscope  the  refracting 
mechanism  of  the  subject's  eye  is  used  as  a  magnifier, 
the  focal  length  of  which  can  be  varied  with  the 
auxiliary  lenses  mounted  in  the  instrument. 

The  aberrations  of  the  eye  and  the  differences  in 
criteria  as  to  what  constitutes  a  focus  create  a  problem 
in  trying  to  correlate  the  results  obtained  with  sub- 
jective and  objective  methods. 

A  technique  known  as  the  fogging  method  has  been 
developed  for  measuring  the  'zero  level'  of  ac- 
commodation by  manipulating  the  stimulus  pattern 


Fig  19 


FIG.  18.  Scheiner  principle  with  doubling  as  the  criterion  of  the  retinal  image  being  out  of 
focus. 

FIG.  19.  Scheiner  principle  with  vernier  displacement  as  the  criterion,  .\ssume  that  the  upper 
and  lower  halves  of  the  slit  are  covered  with  polaroid  with  the  axes  at  right  angles,  and  that  the 
two  holes  in  the  viewer  are  also  covered  with  polaroid  with  the  axes  crossed  at  right  angles. 


66() 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


to  force  accommodation  to  this  level.  A  chart  of 
small  letters  is  used  to  control  fixation  and  to  stimu- 
late relaxation  of  accommodation.  Plus  lenses  are 
placed  in  front  of  the  eye  to  relax  accommodation  and 
then  additional  plus  lenses  are  used  to  get  below 
the  level  to  which  accommodation  can  be  relaxed. 
This  'fogs'  the  eye.  Once  the  eye  is  'fogged'  the  plus 
power  is  reduced  to  locate  the  level  at  which  the 
'fog'  ends.  This  represents  the  'zero  lexel'  of  ac- 
commodation. The  measurement  can  be  carried  out 
on  the  two  eyes  at  the  same  time  if  they  have  pre- 
viously been  corrected  for  astigmatism  and  ani- 
sometropia. The  target  is  placed  at  a  distance  of 
about  6  m  to  create  an  awareness  of  distance  and 
also   to   relax   convergence   if  the   test   is   binocular. 

In  many  experiments  it  is  necessary  to  stimulate 
accommodation  to  various  degrees.  For  most  pur- 
poses it  suffices  to  use  a  single  target  with  fine  detail 
to  control  fixation  and  accommodation.  If  the  fine 
detail  is  clearly  visible,  it  is  assumed  that  the  eye  is 
in  focus  for  the  target.  On  the  other  hand  it  is  often 
desirable  to  find  out  whether  the  amount  of  ac- 
commodation in  play  is  leading  or  lagging  behind 
the  stimulus.  One  target  is  used  for  stimulating 
fixation  and  accommodation  and,  if  both  eyes  are 
used,  it  also  serves  as  a  convergence  stimulus.  An 
independent  target  must  be  used  to  measure  the 
amount  of  accommodation  in  play.  This  target  is 
usually  presented  to  one  eye  and  a  beam  splitter  is 
used  to  superimpose  an  image  of  the  measuring 
target  upon  the  stimulus  target.  The  image  of  the 
measuring  target  must  be  adjustable  so  that  it  can 
be  made  to  fall  in  front  of  or  behind  the  stimulus 
target.  A  single  bright  point  source  is  a  good  meas- 
uring target  because  it  cannot  compete  with  the 
more  complex  stimulus  target  in  controlling  ac- 
commodation. This  is  the  principle  underlyins;  the 
stigmatoscope. 

The  threshold  principle  (56;  57,  p.  485;  73)  may 
be  employed  to  avoid  stimulating  accommodation. 
As  long  as  the  target  is  invisible,  it  cannot  stimulate 
accommodation  and  hence  it  is  moved  from  the  out- 
of-focus  position  to  the  just-visible  point.  This  is 
clone  from  both  directions  to  determine  the  limits  of 
the  visible  range.  The  mid-point  is  assumed  to  be 
the  point  of  best  focus. 

It  is  of  interest  to  know  what  happens  to  ac- 
commodation when  the  stimulus  to  accommodation 
is  fixed  and  the  stimulus  to  convergence  is  \aried. 
The  Scheiner  principle  can  be  used  to  good  advantage 
in  this  kind  of  experiment  because  the  changes  in 
accommodation  can  be  continuously  tracked  by 
having  the  subject  adjust  the  target  as  the  stimulus 


to  convergence  is  slowly  decreased  or  increased  CsO- 
The  Scheiner  target  avoids  stimulating  accommoda- 
tion because  the  two  beams  entering  the  pupil  are 
narrow. 

The  skiascope  (32)  or  the  coincidence  optometer 
(30)  may  also  be  used  in  this  kind  of  experiment  to 
measure  the  amount  of  accommodation  in  play. 
However,  the  objective  methods  not  only  present 
criteria  problems  (34).  but  also  the  additional 
stimuli  applied  to  the  retina  may  affect  the  amount 
of  accommodation.  This,  of  course,  is  avoided  in 
the  case  of  the  infrared  skiascope  (16,  49). 

At  low  levels  of  illumination  and  in  the  presence 
of  an  optically  empty  field  of  high  luminance,  ac- 
commodation fluctuates.  Westheimer  (91)  has  been 
successful  in  measuring  these  fluctuations  using 
intermittent  exposure  of  a  Scheiner  target.  The 
subject  reports  at  each  exposure.  This  target  not 
only  measures  accommodation  but  also  controls 
fixation.  Chin  &  Horn  (16)  observed  these  fluctu- 
ations with  an  infrared  skiascope. 

An  optically  empty  field  at  high  luminance  is  pro- 
\ided  by  Knoll's  'blob'  (47)  which  is  a  luminous 
patch  with  diffuse  edges  upon  which  the  subject  can- 
not focus.  It  is  small  enough,  however,  so  that  fixation 
can  be  controlled  by  looking  at  the  center  of  the 
'blob.' 

Another  problem  is  that  of  controlling  convergence 
without  stimulating  accommodation  and  at  the  same 
time  measuring  the  amount  of  accommodation  in 
play.  The  'blob'  described  above  or  a  blurred  line 
(58)  can  serve  as  a  stimulus  to  convergence  and  the 
threshold,  momentary  exposure  or  infrared  principle 
can  be  used  in  measuring  the  accommodation. 

Tracking  rapid  changes  in  accommodation  pre- 
sents a  special  problem.  Changes  in  the  ciliary  muscle 
potential  (76)  can  be  used  as  an  index  of  changes  in 
accommodation.  Photographic  records  of  the  changes 
in  size  of  the  third  Purkinje  image  have  also  been  em- 
ploved  (i,  6,  45).  Campbell's  automatic  recording 
optometer  (15)  should  be  useful  for  this  purpose. 

The  amount  of  accommodation  in  play  at  a  gi\en 
moment  following  a  stimulus  to  a  change  in  accommo- 
dation can  be  measured  by  presenting  a  momentary 
exposure  of  a  measuring  stimulus  at  the  selected  time 
(3.  4.  ?)• 


MECH.\NISM    OF    .\CCOMMOD.\TION 

Intraucular  Mechanism  vj  Aaommndiilion 

Of  all  the  structures  which  might  be  manipulated 
to  focus  the  eye,  the  lens  alone  fulfills  this  role.  Young 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE 


66  I 


(79,  p.  158;  86,  p.  201)  proved  that  the  lens;th  of  the 
eye  does  not  change  with  accommodation  by  placing 
the  front  and  back  of  one  of  his  eyes  between  the  jaws 
of  a  clamp  and  noting  that  there  was  no  change  in 
the  pressure  phosphene  at  the  back  of  the  eye  as  the 
eye  changed  accommodation.  The  X-ray  method  of 
measuring  the  length  of  the  eye  described  abo\e  is 
now  available  for  demonstrating  this  fact.  Young  also 
showed  that  the  cornea  does  not  change  its  curvature 
during  accommodation  by  noting  the  reflections  from 
the  front  surface.  He  also  immersed  his  eye  in  water 
which  has  about  the  same  index  as  the  aqueous  and 
showed  that  the  power  of  accommodation  was  not  im- 
paired. The  final  proof  offered  by  Young  that  the  lens 
alone  pro\ides  the  mechanism  of  accommodation  is 
the  fact  that  the  eye  assumes  a  fixed  focus  when  the 
lens  is  absent. 

Bv  measuring  the  curvature  of  the  front  and  back 
surface  of  the  lens  and  the  distances  of  the  two  surfaces 
from  the  cornea,  Helmholtz  79,  p.  143)  showed  that 
the  lens  increases  in  thickness  and  moves  forward 
slightly,  that  the  curvature  of  the  back  surface  also 
increases  slightly  but  that  the  most  important  change 
is  in  the  curvature  of  the  front  surface  of  the  lens. 

Helmholtz  believed  these  changes  to  be  brought 
aljout  by  a  decrease  in  tension  of  the  zonule  which 
attaches  the  lens  to  the  ciliary  body  surrounding  the 
lens.  The  lens  was  regarded  as  a  pliable  body  enclosed 
in  an  elastic  capsule.  Such  a  body  tends  to  assume  an 
ellipsoidal  form  when  a  centripetal  tension  is  applied 
all  along  its  equator  but  tends  to  assume  a  more 
spherical  form  when  released  from  this  tension. 

Helmholtz  believed  that  the  release  in  tension  on 
the  zonule  required  for  accommodation  is  brought 
about  by  a  contraction  of  the  ciliary  muscle  which  acts 
partly  as  a  sphincter  in  reducing  the  diaineter  of  the 
ciliary  margin  and  partly  as  a  system  of  radial  fibers 
pulling  forward  the  choroid  to  which  it  is  attached. 
This  increases  the  pressure  of  the  vitreous  on  the  back 
side  of  the  lens  and  neutralizes  the  tendency  of  the 
lens  to  bulge  on  its  back  side.  The  mechanical  pressure 
of  the  iris  on  the  peripheral  part  of  the  front  surface 
of  the  lens  would  help  to  increase  the  curvature  of  the 
front  surface. 

Fincham  (26,  p.  42)  has  demonstrated  in  a  patient 
with  aniridia  that  the  mechanical  pressure  of  the  iris 
is  not  an  essential  part  of  the  mechanism  of  accommo- 
dation. In  this  case  he  could  observe  the  decrease  in 
diameter  of  the  lens  and  of  the  margin  of  the  ciliary 
body  during  accommodation.  Finchain  (26,  p.  50) 
has  described  a  person  with  an  eye  in  which  the  lens 
substance  had  been  dissolved  out  of  the  capsule  and 
showed  that  when  the  other  eve  accommodated  the 


tension  on  the  capsule  decreased  and  left  it  free  to 
dangle.  Hensen  &  \'oelkers  (86,  p.  199)  demonstrated 
the  forward  movement  of  the  choroid  during  accom- 
modation by  inserting  a  needle  through  the  sclera  and 
choroid  near  the  ora  serrata  and  then  making  the 
ciliary  muscle  contract.  The  protruding  part  of  the 
needle  moved  backward  as  the  choroid  ino\ed 
forward. 

Young  (86,  p.  208)  demonstrated  that  the  spherical 
aberration  of  the  eye  becomes  partially  corrected 
when  the  eye  accommodates  and  Tscherning  (86, 
p.  211)  used  the  specular  reflections  from  the  front 
surface  of  the  lens  to  demonstrate  that  this  was  due 
to  a  flattening  of  the  peripheral  part  of  the  front  sur- 
face of  the  lens.  The  Helmholtz  theory  had  not  taken 
this  fact  into  account. 

It  remained  for  Fincham  (26,  p.  59)  to  demonstrate 
that  this  is  due  to  a  variation  in  the  thickness  of  the 
capsule.  The  details  of  the  theory  of  how  an  elastic 
ellipsoidal  inembrane  having  zones  of  varying  thick- 
ness from  the  pole  to  the  equator  affects  the  form  of 
the  lens  as  a  result  of  a  change  in  tension  of  the  zonule 
have  not  yet  been  worked  out. 

Fincham  (26,  p.  i  7)  demonstrated  the  elastic  force 
of  the  capsule  by  puncturing  the  lens  and  noting  that 
the  lens  substance  protruded  through  the  capsule. 
Also  by  direct  experiment  Fincham  (26,  p.  24)  demon- 
strated that  the  zonule  behaves  like  an  elastic  mem- 
brane. Furthermore,  when  the  capsule  of  the  lens  is 
removed,  the  lens  assumes  a  form  of  its  own  in  which 
the  decapsulated  lens  is  more  spherical  (26,  p.  65). 

Hess  (26,  p.  52;  79,  p.  398)  observed  that  when  a 
maximal  efTort  of  accommodation  is  made,  tension  on 
the  zonule  is  relaxed  so  that  the  position  of  the  lens 
in  the  eye  is  aflfected  by  gravity.  This  can  be  demon- 
strated by  showing  that  the  amplitude  of  accommo- 
dation is  greater  when  the  head  points  downward  than 
when  it  points  upward.  The  lateral  displacement  of 
the  lens  when  the  head  is  laid  on  one  side  or  the  other 
can  be  demonstrated  entoptically  when  there  exists  a 
small  opacity  near  the  front  or  back  pole  of  the  lens. 
Hess  concluded  from  this  finding  that  it  may  not  be 
necessary  for  the  eye  to  make  a  maximal  contraction 
of  the  ciliary  muscle  in  order  to  relax  the  lens  com- 
pletely from  the  tension  of  the  zonule. 

Lancaster  &  Williams  (53)  have  shown  that  when 
a  maximal  state  of  accommodation  is  maintained  over 
a  period  of  time,  the  lens  develops  a  set  so  that  it  can- 
not immediately  relax  to  the  zero  level  of  accommo- 
dation. It  takes  several  minutes  to  overcome  this  set. 
Lancaster  &  Williams  regarded  this  as  evidence  that 
the  lens  is  completely  released  from  the  tension  of 
the  zonule  at  the  maximum  level  of  accommodation. 


662 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY'    I 


However,  in  some  subjects  the  same  type  of  effect 
occurs  in  lesser  degree  with  submaximal  amounts  of 
accommodation. 

Duane  (21)  showed  that  in  a  youns;  man  it  takes 
the  same  time  for  homatropine  to  begin  to  take  effect 
as  in  an  older  man  and  argued  from  this  that  the  older 
man  has  the  same  excess  of  ciliary  capacity  above  the 
capacity  of  the  lens  to  respond  as  the  young  man. 

It  is  necessary  to  look  in  some  other  direction  to 
ascertain  what  fraction  of  the  total  ciliary  contraction 
a\ailable  is  required  to  produce  a  diopter  of  accom- 
modation at  different  ages.  The  answer  is  to  be  found 
in  the  study  of  the  relation  of  accommodation  to  con- 
vergence as  described  in  the  next  section. 

It  remains  to  be  determined  whether  the  lens  layers 
become  nonpliai)le  one  by  one  from  the  center  out 
with  the  cortical  layers  remaining  relatively  unaffected 
or  whether  all  la\ers  get  progressively  less  pliable 
with  age  but  with  the  hardening  process  more  de- 
veloped at  the  center  than  at  the  cortex.  The  role 
played  by  the  tensile  strength  of  the  indi\-idual  fibers 
must  also  be  considered. 

The  slit  lamp  technique  of  observing  or  photo- 
graphing the  internal  structure  of  the  lens,  if  system- 
atically used  at  different  age  levels,  might  throw  some 
light  on  this  problem.  It  would  also  be  important  to 
study  at  different  ages  the  time  characteristics  of  the 
response  of  the  lens  to  changes  in  tension  of  the 
zonule  (7). 

Ciliary  Muscle  Potential 

Schubert  (76)  has  recently  developed  a  method  for 
detecting  and  recording  a  potential  which  appears  to 
be  generated  by  contraction  of  the  ciliary  muscle. 
One  electrode  is  applied  to  the  .sclera  o\er  the  ciliary 
bodv  and  the  other  to  some  indifferent  part  of  the 
bod\-.  Alpern  (unpublished  observation.s)  has  de- 
scribed the  relation  of  size  of  this  potential  to  the 
amount  of  accommodation  in  play.  This  new  tech- 
nicjue  provides  us  with  an  opportunity  to  study  the 
lag  of  the  response  of  the  lens  behind  the  changes  in  the 
ciliary  mu.scle. 

Innervation  Controlling  Accommodation 

Although  the  ciliary  muscle  is  classified  histologi- 
cally as  a  smooth  muscle  and  the  branch  of  the  third 
nerve  supplying  it  is  identified  as  a  part  of  the  para- 
sympathetic system,  the  ciliary  muscle  behaves  in 
many  ways  like  a  skeletal  muscle.  Marg  et  al.  (64)  have 
demonstrated  that  in  the  cat  the  amount  of  accom- 


modation elicited  in  response  to  a  square  wave  gal- 
\anic  current  applied  to  the  ciliary  ganglion  is  a  func- 
tion both  of  the  strength  of  the  current  and  the  fre- 
quency of  the  stimuli.  A  submaximal  response  of  any 
degree  may  be  elicited. 

Allen  (2)  has  developed  a  method  which  can  be 
used  in  the  cat  or  dog  for  comparing  the  lag  of  the 
lens  and  the  lag  of  the  muscle.  A  faradic  stimulus  is 
applied  to  the  region  of  the  ciliary  muscle.  A  needle 
pushed  through  the  sclera  into  the  choroid  gives  a 
record  of  the  muscle  response,  and  motion  picture 
photography  is  used  to  measure  the  change  in  size  of 
the  third  Purkinje  image. 

When  the  eyes  accommodate,  they  also  converge 
and  the  pupils  constrict.  This  is  known  as  the  triad 
response.  The  convergence  part  of  the  triad  response 
is  called  accommodative  convergence  because  it  is 
associated  with  accommodation.  It  has  to  be  differen- 
tiated from  fusional  convergence  which  is  a  different 
kind  of  response. 

Allen  (i)  devised  an  arrangement  for  suddenly 
switching  from  a  stimulus  at  one  distance  to  a  stimulus 
at  a  different  distance.  The  stimuli  were  presented  to 
one  eye  only  and  the  ensuing  accommodative  and 
convergence  responses  were  tracked  with  recording 
devices.  The  accommodative  response  lags  a  little 
behind  the  convergence  response  (see  fig.  20)  and 
this  may  be  due  to  the  lag  of  the  lens  behind  the  re- 
sponse of  the  ciliary  muscle.  The  results  confirm  the 
notion  that  the  two  types  of  responses  are  initiated 
through  a  common  center. 

The  triad  response  is  probably  always  brought  into 
plav  bv  voluntary  effort  and  it  behaves  like  a  postural 
adjustment  of  the  arm  which  may  be  raised  to  a  given 
level  and  held  in  that  posture  while  the  subject  pays 
primarv  attention  to  .some  other  aspect  of  his  behavior. 
Under  normal  conditions  of  use  of  the  eyes,  the  only 


'^.46 

E 
E 
~.53 

UJ 
M 

u 
".73 


CONVERGENCE 


-^  3rd    PURKINJE     IMAGE  SIZES 


1 I I 1 1 L 


O 


.5  1.0 

TIME     (SECONDS) 


IS 


FIG.  20.  Accommodative  and  accommodative  convergence 
responses  to  a  change  in  the  stimulus  to  accommodation.  The 
points  represent  sizes  of  the  third  Purkinje  image  measured  in 
successive  frames  of  a  motion  picture  record  occurring  at  a 
rate  of  64  per  sec.  [From  Allen  (i).] 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE 


663 


awareness  of  voluntary  effort  to  readjust  the  triad 
mechanism  is  an  awareness  of  switching  attention 
from  an  object  percei\eci  to  be  located  at  one  distance 
to  an  object  perceived  to  be  located  at  another  dis- 
tance (60).  As  a  matter  of  fact  a  readjustment  of  the 
triad  mechanism  can  be  evoked  when  the  subject  is 
in  total  darkness  by  ha\ing  him  switch  his  attention 
from  an  imaginary  far  point  to  an  imaginary  near 
point  (47).  The  problem  of  focusing  the  eye,  however, 
is  not  quite  this  simple.  If  the  subject  starts  by  paying 
attention  to  a  given  object,  then  covers  one  eye  and 
places  a  minus  or  plus  lens  in  front  of  the  other  to 
throw  the  image  out  of  focus,  and  then  concentrates 
on  the  object  or  attempts  to  clear  up  the  blur,  the 
object  eventually  comes  into  focus  without  the  subject 
perceiving  any  change  in  distance.  We  have  yet  to 
learn  whether  this  response  is  a  result  of  the  voluntary 
effort  to  clear  up  the  blur  or  a  reflex  response  to  the 
blur  which  is  akin  to  the  reaction  of  an  automatic 
focusing  device.  Regardless  of  whether  this  accommo- 
dative response  to  blur  is  voluntary  or  reflex,  it  ap- 
pears to  involve  the  same  tie-up  with  convergence  and 
pupillary  constriction  as  the  accommodative  response 
to  a  change  in  the  distance  of  attention.  Considerable 
attention  is  being  devoted  today  to  the  problem  of 
whether  the  eye  can  detect  ahead  of  time  from  some 
aspect  of  a  blurred  image  whether  to  increase  or  de- 
crease accommodation  to  clear  up  the  blur.  Fincham 
(28,  29)  has  investigated  the  response  to  blur  and  has 
found  evidence  that  the  colored  fringes  on  the  target 
resulting  froin  chromatic  aberration  determine  the 
direction  of  the  response.  Allen  (6)  has  also  investi- 
gated what  determines  the  direction  of  the  response 
when  the  subject  is  confronted  with  a  blurred  stimulus 
with  all  cues  of  distance  eliminated.  In  19  of  the  20 
trials  in  which  the  response  was  recorded,  the  subject's 
first  response  was  in  the  right  direction  and  in  only 
one  trial  did  he  make  an  initial  response  in  the  wrong 
direction  which  had  to  be  corrected  by  a  second 
adjustment.  Astigmatism  as  well  as  chromatic  aberra- 
tion could  provide  the  subject  with  a  cue  as  to  the 
right  direction. 

It  is  possible  that  the  cortical  center  which  controls 
the  triad  response  transmits  impulses  simultaneously 
to  the  centers  in  the  midbrain  controlling  convergence, 
accommodation  and  pupil  constriction.  On  the  other 
hand  it  is  entirely  possible  that  the  triad  innervation 
from  the  cortex  is  first  transmitted  to  the  center  con- 
trolling accommodation  and  relayed  from  there  to 
the  centers  controlling  convergence  and  pupil  con- 
striction. This  is  possible  because  the  brain-stem 
center    controlling    accommodation    never    responds 


without  the  simultaneous  occurrence  of  a  convergence 
and  a  pupillary  response.  However,  the  accommoda- 
tive response  could  not  be  mediated  either  through 
the  brain-stem  center  controlling  convergence  or 
through  the  center  controlling  pupillary  constriction 
becau.se  these  same  centers  mediate  other  types  of 
pupillary  and  convergence  responses  which  are  not 
associated  with  accommodation. 

The  same  center  in  the  midbrain  which  mediates 
the  pupillary  part  of  the  triad  response  also  mediates 
the  pupillary  response  to  light.  It  is  assumed  to  be 
located  in  the  Edinger-Westphal  nucleus. 

Furthermore  the  brain-stem  center  which  mediates 
the  convergence  part  of  the  triad  response  also  medi- 
ates fusional  convergence.  Both  types  of  convergence 
induce  an  excycloductive  movement  of  the  two  eyes, 
i.e.  the  two  eyes  rotate  around  their  lines  of  sight  with 
the  tops  of  the  eyes  turning  outward.  This  indicates 
that  both  types  of  convergence  are  mediated  by  the 
same  mechanism  in  the  brain  stem.  There  is  no  cyclo- 
rotational  movement  associated  with  simple  conjugate 
movements  of  the  eye  to  the  right  or  left  (5). 

On  the  other  hand  there  is  ample  evidence  that  the 
brain-stem  mechanism  for  accommodative  and  fu- 
sional convergence  receives  innervation  from  two 
different  cortical  centers  in  mediating  these  two  types 
of  responses.  Fusional  convergence  is  a  reflex  response 
to  stimulation  of  disparate  points  of  the  two  retinas. 
This  is  probably  a  feed-back  type  of  response  in  which 
the  eyes  constantly  tend  to  drift  to  the  phoria  position, 
i.e.  that  which  they  would  assume  if  one  eye  were 
placed  under  a  cover,  but  are  brought  back  to  the 
fusion  position  by  the  retinal  disparity  resulting  from 
the  drift. 

Knoll  (46)  and  Marg  &  Morgan  (61,  62)  have 
demonstrated  that  a  marked  pupil  constriction  is 
associated  with  a  change  in  accommodation  and  ac- 
commodative convergence,  but  the  pupil  response 
associated  with  fusional  convergence  is  almost  negligi- 
ble. The  very  fact  that  fusional  convergence  can  be 
manipulated  without  affecting  accommodative  con- 
vergence is  in  itself  evidence  that  it  involves  a  separate 
cortical  mechanism. 

Reese  &  Hofstetter  (7;^)  have  reported  a  case 
in  which  accommodation  and  accommodative  con- 
vergence were  absent,  but  positive  fusional  conver- 
gence was  still  operative.  An  ordinary  concomitant 
squinter  may  have  a  normal  amount  of  accommoda- 
tive convergence  but  a  total  absence  of  fusional  con- 
vergence. 

The  relationship  of  accommodative  and  fusional 
convergence  (8,  31,  33)  at  various  levels  of  accommo- 


664 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


Q.  10  — 


z 
o 


o 
o 

o 


SPIKE 


LIMIT    OF 
POS.FUS.CON. 


0  50  100 

CONVERGENCE    (CENTRADS) 


FIG.  -i I .  Relation  between  accommodation,  accommodati\e 
convergence  and  positive  and  negative  fusional  convergence  in 
a  normal  20-year-o]d  subject. 


dation  is  shown  in  figure  2 1 .  At  each  level  of  accommo- 
dation the  phoria  position  of  the  two  eyes  represents 
the  position  of  rest  that  the  eyes  assume  when  one  of 
them  is  covered.  At  the  same  level  of  accommodation 
the  two  eyes  can  be  made  to  converge  toward  or 
diverge  away  from  this  position  of  rest  in  response  to 
stimuli  upon  which  the  two  eyes  can  fu.se,  and  the 
extent  to  which  this  can  be  done  without  changing 
accommodation  gives  us  the  limits  of  fusional  con- 
vergence. The  phoria  line  shows  the  relation  between 
accommodation  and  accommodative  convergence 
when  the  eyes  are  freed  from  fusion  stimuli.  The  re- 
ciprocal of  the  slope  of  this  line  is  called  the  ACA  ratio. 
The  'spike'  at  the  maximum  level  of  accommodation 
which  indicates  that  an  increased  amount  of  con- 
vergence can  develop  at  that  level  was  regarded  by 
van  der  Hoeve  &  Flieringa  (87)  as  evidence  that  the 
ciliary  mu.scle  can  keep  on  responding  after  the  lens 
reaches  its  limit  to  respond,  van  Hoven  (unpublished 
observations)  has  shown  in  six  subjects  that  this  is  not 
the  case.  He  paralyzed  one  eye  with  homatropine  and 
when  the  eye  partially  recovered,  he  measured  the 
phoria  at  various  levels  of  accommodation  for  each  of 
the  two  eyes  and  he  also  measured  the  amplitude  of 
accommodation  of  each  eye  by  measuring  the  ac- 
commodative response  at  the  maximum  level  of  con- 
vergence. He  showed  that  the  amplitude  is  strictly 
proportional  to  the  ACA  ratio.  This  means  that  the 
lens  is  responding  to  changes  in  contraction  of  the 
ciliary  muscle  at  the  maximum  level  of  accommoda- 
tion. Hence  it  is  the  muscle  and  not  the  lens  which 
determines  the  maximum  le\'el  of  accommodation. 
Fincham  (30)  independently  performed  the  same 
kind  of  experiment  except  that  he  has  also  used 
physostigmine.  This  gives  a  hypermaximal  spike 
which  proves  that  the  limit  lies  with  the  muscle  or  its 
innervation  but  not  with  the  lens. 


One  can  explain  the  "spike"  by  assuming  that  the 
ciliary  muscle  reaches  the  limit  of  its  capacity  to  con- 
tract before  the  cortical  center  controlling  the  triad 
responses  reaches  the  limit  of  its  capacity  to  initiate 
impulses. 

The  effect  of  age  upon  the  ACA  ratio  is  important 
(8,  18,  30,  40).  If  the  ACA  were  proportional  to  the 
amplitude  it  ought  to  change  as  the  amplitude 
changes  with  age.  The  studies  made  so  far  have  not 
confirmed  this  relation.  This  raises  the  question 
whether  the  connection  between  accommodation  and 
convergence  is  a  matter  of  habit  instead  of  dependence 
on  some  fixed  anatomical  arrangement. 

Morgan  &  Olmsted  (67)  have  shown  that  stimula- 
tion of  the  sympathetic  supply  to  the  eye  produces  a 
relaxation  of  accommodation  to  the  extent  of  about 
0.75  D  below  what  is  commonly  regarded  to  be  the 
zero  level.  Morgan  (66)  believed  this  eflfect  to  be 
mediated  by  a  change  in  blood  volume  of  the  ciliary 
body,  but  Melton  et  al.  (65)  have  demonstrated  in  a 
ijloodless  eye  that  the  eflTect  is  still  obtained.  The 
mechanism  involved  has  yet  to  be  identified. 

Night  and  Skv  Mvnjna 

In  total  darkness  and  in  the  absence  of  a  stimulus 
to  accommodation  the  refractive  state  of  the  eye  comes 
to  rest  at  a  higher  level  than  occurs  when  the  eyes  are 
looking  at  a  test  chart  at  20  ft.  (47;  48;  56;  57,  p.  485; 
71 ;  72;  84).  The  effect  of  low  levels  of  illumination  can 
partly  be  explained  (84,  89)  by  the  aberrations  of  the 
eye,  but  these  explanations  do  not  apply  in  sky 
myopia  (92).  In  sky  myopia  as  well  as  in  night  myopia 
an  increase  in  accommodation  of  about  i  D  above  its 
zero  level  is  found  to  exist.  Moreover,  VVestheimer 
(gi)  has  shown  that  under  these  conditions  the  ac- 
commodation is  not  fixed  but  exhibits  slow  oscilla- 
tions up  to  a  diopter  in  amplitude.  These  fluctuations 
have  also  been  reported  by  Campbell  (14)  and  by 
Chin  &  Horn  (16). 


VISU.\L    FIELD 

The  visual  field  of  a  gi\en  eye  is  a  conical  space 
with  its  apex  at  the  center  of  the  entrance-pupil  which 
contains  the  chief  rays  for  all  parts  of  the  retina  that 
respond  to  light.  In  the  ordinary  use  of  the  eyes  a  part 
of  the  field  of  view  is  cut  off  h\  the  nose,  eyebrow  and 
cheek. 

The  direction  of  a  point  in  the  field  of  \  icw  may  be 
specified  in  terms  of  its  radial  direction  and  eccen- 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE  66=^ 


tricity  from  the  primary  line  of  sight.  Objects  in  the 
zero  radial  direction  lie  in  the  plane  of  regard  to  the 
left  of  the  line  of  sight.  Other  radial  directions  are  dis- 
placed clockwise  around  the  line  of  sight  and  specified 
in  degrees  from  o  to  360.  This  is  the  same  kind  of 
notation  as  that  used  for  cylinder  axes  mounted  in 
front  of  the  eyes.  Eccentricity  represents  degrees  be- 
tween the  primary  and  secondary  lines  of  sight. 

The  limits  of  the  visual  fields  for  the  right  and  the 
left  eye  are  shown  in  figure  22.  The  combination  of 
the  two  monocular  fields  with  their  centers  coinciding 
represents  the  binocular  visual  field. 


RETINAL    ILLUMINANCE 

In  order  for  a  person  to  see,  it  is  necessary  for 
the  photoreceptors  to  react  to  the  light  falling  on  them 
b>  generating  impulses  which  can  be  transmitted 
to  the  brain.  The  response  of  the  photoreceptor  is  de- 
pendent not  only  on  the  amount  of  light  directed  to- 
ward it  from  the  e.xit-pupil  but  also  upon  its  struc- 
ture and  orientation  with  respect  to  the  exit-pupil. 
The  amount  of  light  falling  upon  a  given  photorecep- 
tor is  dependent  upon  the  amount  of  light  admitted 
into  the  eye  from  the  corresponding  part  of  the  field 
of  view  and  upon  the  transmittance  of  the  eye.  We 
have  in  the  eye  not  onlv  the  light  which  is  focused  by 
the  image-forming  mechanism  at  or  near  the  retina 
but  also  a  certain  amount  of  stray  light.  These  prob- 
lems can  all  be  treated  in  a  quantitative  way,  although 
it  is  necessary  to  introduce  a  few  photometric  con- 
cepts and  units.  The  units  which  have  been  used  be- 
long to  the  meter-kilogram-second  system. 


Light  and  Illuminance 

Light  is  luminous  energy,  and  a  unit  of  this  energy 
is  called  a  talbot.  Illuminance  is  a  term  which  is  used 
to  describe  the  rate  at  which  light  is  falling  on  a  sur- 
face from  all  directions.  The  statement  that  one  lux 
of  illuminance  is  falling  on  a  surface  indicates  that  a 
total  of  one  talbot  of  light  is  falling  each  second  from 
all  directions  on  a  square  meter  of  the  surface. 

Solid  Angle 

The  simplest  way  to  visualize  a  solid  angle  is  to 
consider  a  certain  area  on  the  surface  of  a  sphere. 
This  area  is  said  to  subtend  a  certain  amount  of  solid 


270 


RIGHT 


FIG.   ^2.   Monocular  and  bioncular  \'isual  tields. 


angle  at  the  center  of  the  sphere.  It  is  measured  in 
steradians  and  is  equal  to  the  area  of  the  surface 
divided  by  the  square  of  the  radius. 

Luminance 

Luminance  is  a  term  which  may  be  used  to  describe 
the  rate  at  which  light  is  falling  on  the  e\e  from  a 
given  direction.  Consider  any  line  of  sight.  The  lumi- 
nance in  this  direction  as  measured  at  the  center  of 
the  entrance-pupil  represents  the  illuminance  per  unit 
solid  angle  falling  on  a  surface  perpendicular  to  the 
line  of  sight  at  the  center  of  the  entrance-pupil.  One 
unit  of  luminance  is  equal  to  one  lux  of  illuminance 
per  steradian  of  solid  angle. 

Retiiuil  Illuminance 

The  illuminance  E  (luxes)  at  a  given  point  on  the 
retina  corresponding  to  a  given  direction  in  the  field 
of  view  is  given  by  the  following  equation, 

E  =  BtA  cos  d/k 

where 

B  =  luminance  in  the  given  direction  (nits), 
/  =  transmittance  of  the  eye, 
A  =  area  of  the  pupil  (//r), 

9  =  angle  of  incidence  of  the  chief  ray  at  the  plane 
of  the  pupil,  and 


666 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


A  =  a   constant   which   represents   the   ratio   of  a 
given  area  (m-)  of  the  retinal  image  to  the 
corresponding  solid  angle  (steradians)  in  the 
visual  field. 
Since  /  and  k  are  constants  and  since  cos  6  in  the 
usual  case  may   be  assumed   to   be  equal   to   unity, 
Troland  (85)  proposed  a  unit  of  retinal  illuminance 
which  he  called  the  photon  (now  known  as  the  tro- 
land). The  number  of  trolands  at  a  given  point  on  the 
retina  is  equal  to  the  number  of  nits  multiplied  by  the 
area  of  the  pupil  in  square  millimeters. 

This  unit  is  useful  in  an  experiment  in  which  an 
artificial  pupil  is  used,  but  should  not  be  used  when 
pupils  of  different  sizes  are  used  unless  the  proper 
allowance  is  made  for  the  Stiles-Crawford  effect. 

Transmittance  of  the  Eye 

The  spectral  transmittance  of  the  eye  from  the 
cornea  to  the  retina  as  measured  by  Ludvigh  & 
McCarthy  (59)  is  shown  by  the  dots  in  figure  23. 
This  includes  consideration  of  the  Icsses  by  reflection 
and  scatter  at  the  surfaces  as  well  as  the  losses  by  alj- 
sorption  and  scatter  in  the  media.  In  considering 
foveal  vision  it  is  necessary  to  pay  attention  to  the 
brown  or  yellowish  spot  of  macular  pigment  covering 
the  central  14-degree  region  of  the  retina  which  is 
called  the  macula  lutea.  The  transmittance  of  the 
macular  pigment  in  this  region  according  to  VVald 
(88)  is  given  by  the  circles  in  figure  23  and  the  curve 
represents    the    product    of    the    two    transmittances 


500  600 

WAVELENGTH,  rriM 


FIG.  23.  Spectral  transmittance  of  the  ocular  media.  The 
circles  represent  the  transmittance  of  the  macular  pigment,  and 
the  dots  the  transmittance  of  the  media  from  the  cornea  to  the 
retina.  The  curve  represents  total  transmittance.   [From  Judd 

(44)-] 


giving  the  total  transmittance  of  the  ocular  media. 
The  absorption  in  the  ultraviolet  region  depends 
largely  upon  the  lens.  In  an  aphakic  eye  enough  ultra- 
\iolet  reaches  the  retina  so  that  objects  invisible  to  the 
normal  eye  with  ultraviolet  illumination  are  easily 
seen  by  the  aphakic  eye. 

Maxwell's  spot,  which  can  be  seen  when  the  eye 
alternates  fixation  from  a  gray  surface  to  a  purple 
surface  of  the  same  brightness,  is  probably  dependent 
upon  the  macular  pigment.  Walls  &  Mathews  (90) 
believe  it  to  be  a  function  of  the  distribution  of  differ- 
ent kinds  of  photoreceptors. 

Polarization  affects  the  amount  of  light  reaching 
the  retina  as  shown  by  Haidinger's  brushes  which  are 
visible  in  looking  at  the  blue  .sky  through  a  polaroid 
filter.  This  polarization  effect  is  attributed  to  Henle's 
fibers  (22,  p.  806)  which  radiate  from  the  center  of 
the  fovea  and  connect  the  cones  at  the  center  of  the 
fovea  with  bipolar  cells  which  are  displaced  toward 
the  edge  of  the  fovea. 


Stiles-Craivjoril  Effect 

Stiles  &  Crawford  (83)  have  in\estigated  the  rela- 
tive luminous  efficiency  of  rays  entering  different  parts 
of  the  pupil.  The  results  in  the  horizontal  meridian 
in  a  typical  case  are  given  in  figure  24.  Los.ses  from 
reflection  and  scattering  at  the  refracting  surfaces 
and  losses  from  absorption  and  scattering  by  the  media 
niav  contribute  to  this  effect  but  the  most  important 
factor  is  the  angle  of  incidence  at  the  photoreceptors. 
It  is  an  effect  which  involves  cones  but  not  rods  (82) 
which  are  normally  oriented.  It  is  not  affected  by 
polarization  of  the  light  (70).  Phase  diflFerences  in  two 
beams  entering  different  parts  of  the  pupil  (25)  do 
not  affect  the  efficiency  of  the  beams  when  thev  are 
combined  again  at  the  retina,  and  hence  the  eflfect 
can  be  treated  as  if  it  were  produced  by  a  gradient 
filter  covering  the  pupil  which  has  a  high  transmit- 
tance at  the  center  which  tapers  off  at  the  edge. 


Stra\  Li§lit  in  the  Eye 

There  are  .several  sources  of  stray  light  in  the  eye 
(10,  35,  36,  55,  81):  fl)  diffusion  through  the  sclera  and 
iris  (10,  75);  0  flare  in  the  optical  system  (55),  in- 
cluding the  light  reflected  from  the  iris  to  the  cornea 
and  thence  through  the  pupil  to  the  retina,  and  also 
part  of  the  light  difl"usely  reflected  by  the  retina 
which  mav  be  reflected  back  toward  the  retina  by  one 


THE    IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EYE 


667 


4     3    2     10    12    3    4mm. 
DISTANCE  FROM  THE  CENTER 
OF  THE  PUPIL 

FIG.  24.  The  Stiles-Crawford  effect.  Data  for  the  horizontal 
meridian  of  the  right  eye  of  B.H.C.  (81).  [From  Fry  (37).] 


of  the  refracting  surfaces;  <)  scatter  by  the  media 
(12,  35,  36,  41,  55,  81)  including  the  halos(22,  p.  801) 
produced  by  diffraction  associated  with  the  micro- 
structure  of  the  lens  and  cornea;  (f)  diffuse  reflection 
from  the  pigment  epithelium,  choroid  and  .sclera 
(19,  34),  this  light  stimulating  the  photoreceptors  in 
passing  back  through  the  retina  (halation),  and  then 
passing  through  the  vitreous  to  the  other  parts  of  the 
retina  (after  reaching  some  other  part  of  the  retina  a 
part  of  the  light  may  be  further  reflected);  e)  fluores- 
cence of  the  lens  (22,  p.  820)  and  the  retina  (22, 
p.  821)  when  exposed  to  ultraviolet  light;  and  /)  bio- 
luminescence  in  the  photoreceptors  which  Judd 
(44,  69)  has  proposed  may  cause  one  of  the  images  in 
the  sequence  following  a  flash  of  light.  [The  'blue  arcs' 
associated  with  the  passage  of  impulses  along  ganglion 
cell  axons  across  the  retina  are  explained  by  some 
writers  as  a  form  of  electroluminescence,  but  the  evi- 
dence favors  the  direct  electrical  excitation  of  the 
underlying  eleinents  (68).] 

In  the  actual  use  of  the  eyes  a  person  is  most  likely 
to  run  into  the  problem  of  stray  light  in  connection 
with  the  impairing  effect  of  a  peripheral  glare  source 
on  foveal  vision.  The  effect  of  the  glare  source  on  a 
given  test  object  can  be  compared  with  the  effect  of 
a  patch  of  veiling  luminance  superimposed  on  the  test 
object  (41,  42,  80).  It  is  satisfactory  to  assume  that 
this  effect  is  mediated  by  stray  light  (12,  35,  36,  81) 
and  hence  the  luminance  of  the  veiling  patch  may  be 
used  as  a  measure  of  the  stray  light.  In  figure  25  the 
ratio  of  the  veiling  luminance  By  (nits)  to  the  illumi- 


nance E  (luxes)  in  the  plane  of  the  pupil  produced 
by  a  glare  .source  is  plotted  as  a  function  of  the  angle 
d  of  the  glare  source  from  the  primary  line  of  sight. 
Stray  light  is  especially  important  in  interpreting 
the  results  of  electroretinography  (11,  13,  38,  93)  and 
pupillography  (39).  If  the  eye  is  exposed  to  a  small 
bright  stimulus,  the  electrical  potentials  or  the 
pupillary  response  produced  by  the  millions  of  ele- 
ments feebly  stimulated  by  stray  light  may  completely 
mask  the  response  of  the  few  elements  stimulated  by 
focused  light. 

BLUR    OF    RETINAL    IMAGE 

The  retinal  image  can  be  treated  either  as  a  geo- 
metrical or  as  a  physical  image.  In  treating  the  image 
of  a  monochromatic  point  source  as  a  geometrical 
image,  one  assumes  that  the  rays  from  the  point  source 
which  pass  through  the  pupil  are  uniforinly  distrib- 
uted across  the  pupil.  These  rays  can  be  traced  to  the 
retina  and  the  illutriinance  at  any  part  of  the  image 
is  proportional  to  the  concentration  of  rays  at  that 
point. 

In  treating  the  retinal  image  as  a  physical  image, 
it  is  assumed  that  the  light  entering  the  eye  is  propa- 
gated in  the  form  of  waves  and  diffraction  is  taken 
into  consideration. 

The  concept  of  a  blur  circle  produced  by  throwing 
the  eye  out  of  focus  and  of  a  ijlur  ellipse  produced  by 
astigmatism  is  based  upon  geometrical  imagery.  The 


100 


05°      I" 


e  (DEGREES) 


FIG.  25.  The  equivalent  veiling  brightness  (Bv)  of  a  glare 
source  at  various  glare  angles  (f).  [From  Fry  (36).] 


668 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


AR 


_ 

+  100 

- 

0 

- 

^ 

^ 

) 

-1.00 

- 

/ 

/ 

2  00 

/ 

3C 

0 

400 

500 

600 

700      8a 

X 


FIG.  26.  Chromatic  aberration  data  of  Wald  &  Griffcn 
(89),  cited  by  Fry  (37).  AR  is  the  distance  from  the  cornea  to 
the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina.  X  represents  wave  length. 
[From  Fry  (37).] 


-6  -5  -4  -3  -2  -I 

BEST  SPECTACLE  CORRECTION  FOR  ZONE.  DIOPTERS 


FIG.  27.  Spherical  aberration  of  an  eye  with  various  amounts 
of  accommodation  in  play.  [From  Koomen  et  al.  (52).] 


light  is  assumed  to  be  uniformly  distributed  over  the 
blur  circle  or  blur  ellipse.  This  approach  to  the  prob- 
lem of  throwing  the  eye  out  of  focus  is  usually  quite 
adequate. 

Geometrical  ray  tracing  is  also  adequate  for 
describing  and  defining  aberrations,  but  in  order  to 
evaluate  the  effect  of  aberrations  upon  the  distribu- 
tion of  illuminance  in  the  retinal  image  it  is  necessary 
to  deal  with  the  physical  image. 

The  a.xial  chromatic  aberration  of  the  eye  can  i^e 
measured  by  locating  the  conjugate  focus  of  the  retina 
for  different  wavelengths  of  light.  Figure  26  gives  the 
average  data  for  the  seven  subjects  of  Wald  &  Griffin 
(89)  expressed  in  terms  of  an  eye  focused  on  a  yellow 
(589  mfx)  point  at  infinit\-. 

The  spherical  aberration  of  the  eye  can  be  expressed 
in  terms  of  the  conjugate  foci  of  the  retina  for  different 
zones   of   the    pupil.    Figure    27    shows    the   data    of 


Koomen  et  al.  (52)  for  a  typical  eye  for  several  dif- 
ferent amounts  of  accommodation.  Many  arrange- 
ments (9,  34,  43,  51,  52)  have  been  used  for  measuring 
the  spherical  aberration  of  the  eye. 

Chromatic  dispersion  (37,  p.  89)  of  the  eye  is 
dependent  upon  the  axial  chromatic  aberration  of  the 
eye  and  the  lateral  displacement  of  the  pupil  from  the 
incident  ray  directed  through  the  primary  nodal 
point  of  the  eye  (see  fig.  28).  Blur  produced  by  chro- 
matic dispersion  is  akin  to  astigmatism  in  being 
radially  asymmetrical.  Other  aberrations  in  the 
human  eye,  such  as  coma  and  radial  and  irregular 
astigmatism,  have  not  been  extensively  studied. 

Figure  29  shows  the  effect  of  diffraction  (37,  p.  57) 
upon  the  image  of  a  monochromatic  point  source  in 
an  eye  free  from  aberrations  and  astigmatism  and  in 
perfect  focus.  The  geoinetrical  image  would  be  a  point. 
Reducing  the  size  of  the  pupil  increases  the  blur  due 
to  diffraction  and  minimizes  the  effect  of  being  out  of 
focus  and  the  effects  of  chromatic  and  spherical  aber- 
ration. A  pupil  size  of  about  4  mm  yields  maximum 
sharpness  of  \ision  in  an  eve  which  is  well-focused 

(17)- 

Once  the  distriljution  of  illuminance  for  a  single 
point  is  known,  the  distribution  of  illuminance  on  the 


CHROMATIC 
DISPERSION 


OPTIC 
AXIS 


BLUE 


AXIAL 
CHROMATIC  ABERRATION 


FIG.  28.  Dependence  of  chromatic  dispersion  on  axial  chro- 
matic aberration  and  lateral  displacement  of  the  pupil. 


FIG.  29.  Disli  iliution  iit  ilhiniinance  across  the  center  of  the 
phvsical  image  of  a  monochromatic  point  source  in  an  eye  free 
from  spherical  aberration  and  astigmatism  and  focused  for 
the  sharpest  possible  image. 


THE     IMAGE-FORMING    MECHANISM    OF    THE    EVE 


669 


on 


POINT 


LINE 


BORDER 


FIG.  30.  Distributions  of  illuminance  in  the  images  of  a  point, 
a  line  and  a  border,  representing  the  same  degree  of  blur. 


retina  may  be  calculated  for  any  pattern.  The  index 
of  blur  <j)  proposed  by  Fry  &  Cobb  (37,  p.  33)  pro- 
vides a  method  of  specifying  the  amount  of  blur 
regardless  of  how  it  is  caused.  Figure  30  illustrates  the 
distribution  of  retinal  luminance  across  the  images  of 
a  point,  a  line  and  a  border.  Although  4>  may  be  de- 
fined in  terms  of  any  one  of  these  images,  its  meaning 
is  best  comprehended  in  the  case  of  a  line.  It  repre- 
sents the  ratio  of  the  area  under  the  curve  to  the  height 
of  the  central  ordinate.  <t>  has  the  advantage  that  for 
any  condition  of  observation  it  can  be  measured  ex- 
perimentally without  analyzing  the  factors  that 
contribute  to  it. 


ENTOPTIC    PHENOMENA 

Entoptic  phenomena  include  the  shadows  on  the 
retina  of  opaque  structures  inside  the  eye  and   the 


nonuniformities  in  the  illumination  of  the  retina  pro- 
duced b\  nonuniformities  of  index  and  surface 
cur\ature. 

A  white  surface  \iewed  through  a  pinhole  held 
near  the  primary  focal  point  produces  a  shadow  of  the 
iris  on  the  retina.  The  irregularities  in  the  margin  are 
clearly  visible  and  changes  in  the  size  of  the  pupil 
can  be  observed  directly.  The  Broca  pupillometer 
(57,  p.  237)  is  based  on  this  method  of  viewing  the 
pupil.  This  method  of  viewing  also  makes  visible  spots 
and  folds  in  the  cornea,  star  figures  and  incipient 
cataracts  in  the  lens,  and  opacities  in  the  \itreous  body 
which  give  rise  to  the  muscae  volitantes.  One  can  also 
see  images  of  the  blood  corpuscles  in  the  retinal  capil- 
laries as  white  spots.  If  the  pinhsle  is  oscillated  back 
and  forth,  one  can  observe  shadows  of  the  large  and 
also  the  minute  blood  \essels.  The  larger  vessels  form 
a  branched  tree  known  as  the  Purkinje  figure. 

This  Purkinje  figure  formed  by  the  blood  vessels 
can  also  be  \iewed  !:>>  illuminating  a  spot  on  the 
sclera  or  by  forming  a  small  bright  image  on  the 
peripheral  retina.  By  using  two  point  sources,  two 
shadows  can  be  produced  whose  angular  separation 
at  the  second  nodal  point  can  be  measured.  From  this 
one  can  determine  the  distance  of  the  vessel  in  front 
of  the  photosensitive  elements. 

Since  the  vessels  move  with  the  photosensitive  ele- 
ments, their  shadows  are  not  affected  by  the 
micronystagmoid  movements  of  the  eye;  this  pro- 
\ides  a  means  of  studying  adaptation  without  in- 
vohing  micronvstagmoid  mosements. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Allen,  M.  J.  Am.  J.  Opiom.  26:  279,  1949. 

2.  Allen,  M.J.  Am.  J.  Opiam.  27:  287,  1950. 

3.  Allen,  M.  J.  Am.  J.  OpIom.  30:  78,  1953. 

4.  Allen,  M.J.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  30:  393,  1953. 

5.  .Allen,  M.  J.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  31  :  297,  1954. 

6.  .Allen,  M.  J.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  32:  422,  1955. 

7.  Allen,  M.  J.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  33:  201,  1956. 

8.  Alpern,  M.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  27:  491,  1950. 

9.  Ames,  A.  Jr.  and  C.  A.   Proctor.  J.   Opt.  Soc.  Am.   5:  22, 
1921. 

10.  Bartlev,   S.   H.    Vision,  A  Sludy  nf  lis   Basis.   Neu-   York: 
Van  Nostrand,  1941,  p.  57. 

1 1.  Bovnton,  R.  M,  J.  Dpi.  Soc.  Am.  43:  442,  1953. 

12.  Bovnton,  R    M.,  J.  M.  Enoch  and  W.  R.  Bush.  J.  Opi. 
Soc.  Am.  44:879,  1954. 

13.  Bovnton,  R.  M.   and  L.   .A.   Riggs.  J.   E.xper.   Psychol.  42: 

■^'7.  I95I- 

14.  Campbell,  F.  W.  Bnl.  orlhopl.  J.  1 1  :  13,  1954. 
X5.   Campbell,  F.  W.J.  Physiol.  133:  31,  1956. 


16.  Chin,  N.  B.  and  R.  E.  Horn.  J.  OpI.  Soc.  Am.  46:  60, 
'956. 

17.  Cobb,  P.  W.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  36:  335,  1915. 

18.  Davis,  G.J.  and  F.  W.  Jobe.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  34:  16,  1957. 

19.  Di  Francia,  G.  T.  and  L.  Ronchi.  J.  OpI.  Soc.  Am.  42: 
782,  1952. 

20.  DoNDERS,  F.  C.  .Accommodalion  and  Rejraction  oj  Ihe  Eye, 
translated  by  \V.  D.  Moore.  London:  New  Sydenham 
Society,  1864,  p.  204. 

21.  DuANE,  A.  Am.  J.  Ophlh.  8:  196,  1925. 

22.  Duke-Elder,  \V.  S.  Texl-book  of  Ophlhalmolog\.  St.  Louis: 
Mosby,  1934,  vol.  I. 

23.  Duke-Elder,  \V.  S.  Texl-book  oj  Ophlhalmology.  St.  Louis: 
Mosby,  1949,  vol.  IV. 

24.  Emsley,  H.  H.  Visual  Opiics  (5th  ed.).  London:  Hatton, 
1952,  vol.  \,  p.  257. 

25.  Enoch,  J.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  48:  392,  1958. 

26.  Finch  AM,  E.  F.  1  he  Mechanism  of  Accommodation.  London: 
Pullman,  1937. 


670 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


27.  FiNCHAM,  E.  F.  Proc.  Phys.  Soc.  London  49:  456,  1937. 

28.  Finch  AM,  E.  F.  Bril.  J.  Ophth.  35:  381,  1951. 

29.  Fincham,  E.  F.  1  romactions  of  the  Internatwnal  Optical 
Congress.  London:  Brit.  Opt.  A.,  1951,  p.  105. 

30.  Fincham,  E.  F.  J.  Physiol.  128:  91,  1955. 

31.  Fry,  G.  a.  Am.  J.  Oplorn.  16:  325,  1939. 

32.  Fry,  G.  a.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  18;  306,  1941. 

33.  Fry,  G.  a.  Optometnc  Weekly  34:  153,  1943. 

34.  Fry,  G.  a.  The  0-Eye-O  15  (Autumn):  8,  1949. 

35.  Fry,  G.  A.  Ilium.  Eniy.  49:  98,  1954. 

36.  Fry,  G.  A.  Physiological  Bases  of  Disability  Glare.  Ctompte 
Rendu,  Commission  Internationale  dc  lEclairage,  Zurich, 
1955.  ■•4-2,  U-F. 

37.  Fry,  G.  a.  Blur  of  the  Retinal  Iniii«e.  Columbus:  Ohio 
State  Univ.  Press,  1955. 

38.  Fry,  G.  a.  and  S.  H.  Hartley.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.  1 1 1  :  335, 

1935- 

39.  Fugate,  J.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  44:  771,  1954. 

40.  HiRSCH,  M.  J.,  M.  Alpern  and  H.  I.  Shultz.  Am.  J. 
Optom.  25:  535,  1948. 

41.  HoLLADAY,  L.  L.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  12;  271,  1926. 

42.  HoLLADAY,  L.  L.  J.  opt.  Soc.  Am.  14:  I,  1927. 

43.  IvANOFF,  I.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  46:  901,  1956. 

44.  JuDD,  D.  B.  Colorimetry  (National  Bureau  of  Standards 
Circular  478).  Washington:  U.  S.  Government  Printing 
Office,  1950,  p.  2. 

45.  KiRCHHOF,  H.  .-Im.  J.  Optom.  27:  163,  1950. 

46.  Knoll,  H.  .4m.  J.  Optom.  26:  346,  1949. 

47.  Knoll,  H.  A.  The  Effect  of  Low  Levels  of  Luminance  and 
Freedom  from  Optical  Stimulation  of  Accommodation  upon  the 
Refractive  State  of  the  Eyes  (Dissertation).  Columbus:  Ohio 
State  University,  1950. 

48.  Knoll,  H.  A.  Am.  J.  Optom.  29:  69,  1952. 

49.  Knoll,  H.  A.  Am.  J.  Optom.  30:  346,  1953. 

50.  Knoll,  H.  A.,  R.  Stimson  and  C.  L.  Weeks.  J.  Opt.  Soc. 
Am.  47:  221,  1957. 

51.  KooMEN,  M.  J.,  R.  Scolnick  and  R.  Tousey.  J.  Opt. 
Soc.  Am.  46:  903,  1956. 

52.  KooMEN,  M.,  R.  Tousey  and  R.  Scolnick.  J.  Opt.  .Soc.  .4m. 

39-  370.  1949- 

53.  Lancaster,   W.   B.   and   E.   R.   Williams.    Tr.   Am.   Acad. 

Ophth.:  170,  1914. 

54.  Laurance,  L.  Visual  Optics  and  Siijht  Testing.  London: 
School  of  Optics,  1926,  p.  452. 

55.  Le  Grand,  Y.  Rev.  Opt.  16:  201,  241,  1937. 

56.  Luckiesh,   M.   and   F.   K.    Moss.   Am.  J.    Ophth.   20:   469, 

'937- 

57.  Luckiesh,  M.  and  F.  K.  Moss.   The  Science  of  Seeing.  New 

York:  Van  Nostrand,  1937. 

58.  Luckiesh,  M.  and  F.  K.  Moss.  A..\L.l.  Arch.  Ophth.  23: 
941,  1940. 

59.  Ludvigh,  E.  and  E.  F.  McCarthy.  A..M.A.  Arch.  Ophth. 
20:  37,  1938. 

60.  Marg,  E.  Am.  J.  Optom.  28:  347,  1951. 

61.  Marg,  E.  and  M.  W.  Morgan,  Jr.  Am.  J.  Optom.  26: 
183,  1949- 


62.  Marg,  E.  and  M.  W.  Morgan,  Jr.  Am.  J.  Optom.  27: 
217:  1950. 

63.  Marg,  E.,  J.  Ong  and  D.   Hamasaki.  Am.  J.  Optom.  33: 

3.  1956. 

64.  Marg,  E.,  J.  L.  Reeves  and  W.  E  Wendt.  Am.  J.  Optom. 
3':  127,  1954. 

65.  Melton,  C.  E.,  E.  W.  Purnell  and  G.  A.  Brecher.  Am. 
J.  Ophth.  40  (Pt.  II):  155,  1955. 

66.  Morgan,  M.  W.,  Jr.  Am.  J.  Oplom.  23:  99,  1946. 

67.  Morgan,  M.  W.,  Jr.  and  J.  M.  D.  Olmsted.  Proc.  Soc. 
Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  42  :  61 2,  1 939. 

68.  Newhall,  S.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  27:  165,  1937. 

69.  Newhall,  S.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  28:  177,  1938. 

70.  O'Brien,  B.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  36:  506,  1946. 

71 .  Otero,  J.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  41 :  942,  1951. 

72.  Reese,  E.  E.  and  G.  A.  Fry.  Am.  J.  Optom.  18:  9,  1941. 

73.  Reese,  E.  E.  and  H.  W.  Hofstetter.  Am.  J.  Optom. 
24:  123,  1947. 

74.  RoNCHi,  V.  optics,  the  Science  of  Vision,  translated  by  E. 
Rosen.  New  York:  New  York  Univ.  Press,  1957,  p.  24. 

75.  ScHOUTEN,  J.  F.  h'oninkl.  .Xed.  .ikad.  Wetenschap.,  Proc.  37: 
5 '6,  1934. 

76.  Schubert,  G.  von  Graefes  Arch.  Ophth.  157:  116,  1955. 

77.  Shastid,  T.  H.  Am.  Encyclopedia  Ophth.  1 1 :  8524,  191 7. 

78.  SoRSBY,  A.  A  Short  History  of  Ophthalmology.  London : 
Bale,  1933. 

79.  SouTH.\LL,  J.  P.  C.  (editor).  Helmholtz's  Treatise  on  Physio- 
logical Optics  (translated  from  3rd  German  ed.).  Rochester; 
Opt.  Soc.  Am.,   1924,  vol.   I. 

80.  Stiles,  W.  S.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B.  104:  322,  1929. 

81.  Stiles,   W.    S.    Proc.    Roy.   Soc,   London,   ser.    B.    105:    131, 

1929- 

82.  Stiles,  W.  S.  Proc.  Roy.  .Soc,  London,  ser.  B.  127:  64,  1939. 

83.  Stiles,  W.  S.  and  B.  H.  Crawford.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London, 
ser.  B.  112:  428,   1933. 

84.  Tousey,  R.,  M.  Koomen  and  R.  Scolnick.  J.  Opt.  Soc 
Am.  43:926,  1953. 

85.  Troland,  L.  T.  Principles  of  Psychophysiology.  New  York: 
Van  Nostrand,  1930,  vol.  II,  p.  62. 

86.  Tscherning,  M.  Physiological  Optics,  translated  by  C.  Wei- 
land.  Philadelphia:  Keystone,  1924. 

87.  van  der  Hoeve,  J.  and  H.  J.  Flieringa.  Brit.  J.  Ophth. 
8:  97,  1924. 

88.  Wald,  G.  Science  loi  :  653,  1945. 

89.  Wald,  G.  and  D.  R.  Griffin.  J.   Opt.  Soc  Am.  37:  321, 

'947- 

90.  Walls,  G.  L.  .\nd  R.  W.  Mathews.  .New  .Means  of  Studying 
Color  Blindness  and  Normal  Foveal  Color  Vision.  Berkeley  and 
Los  Angeles:  Univ.  California  Press,  1952. 

91.  Westheimer,  G.  J.  Opt.  .Soc  Am.  47:  714,  1957. 

92.  Whiteside,  T.  C.  D.  and  F.  W.  Campbell.  Flying  Per- 
sonnel Research  Committee,  Report  821,  March,  1953. 

93.  Wirth,  A.  AND  B.  Zetterstrom.  Brit.  J.  Ophth.  38:  257, 

■954- 

94.  Wulfek,  J.  W.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  45:  928,  1955. 


CHAPTER    XXVIII 


The  photoreceptor  process  in  vision' 


GEORGE    WALD      [      Biological  Laboratories,  Harvard  University,  Cambridge,  Massachusetts 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Chemistry  of  Visual  Excitation 

Rhodopsin 

Porphyiopsin 

lodopsin 

Cyanopsin 

Recapitulation 

Role  of  Opsin  in  Visual  Excitation 
Physiological  Correlations 

Absorption     Spectra     and     Spectral     Sensitivity :     Purkinje 
Phenomenon 

Visual    Adaptation    and    the    Bleaching    and    Synthesis    of 
Visual  Pigments 

Vitamin  A  Deficiency  and  Night  Blindness 

Nicotinamide 


CHEMISTRY    OF    VISUAL    EXCITATION 

LIGHT  INITIATES  a  ncrvous  excitation  in  the  outer 
segments  of  the  rods  and  cones  which,  transmitted 
from  one  neuron  to  another  to  centers  in  the  brain, 
ends  in  exciting  visual  sensations.  To  achieve  this 
resuh  probably  the  whole  apparatus  must  be  thrown 
into  activity;  yet  all  of  it  waits  upon  and,  to  a  degree, 
retains  the  impress  of  the  primary  processes  of  ex- 
citation in  the  receptor  cells. 

The  general  arrangement  of  these  processes  is  clear 
from  first  principles.  Light  to  have  any  effect,  chemi- 
cal or  physical,  must  be  absorbed.  The  rods  and 
cones  must  therefore  contain  substances  which  absorb 
visible  light — hence  pigments — and  are  changed 
thereby  so  as  to  yield  a  nervous  excitation.  The  photo- 

'  The  investigations  from  this  laboratory  were  supported  in 
part  by  the  Rockefeller  Foundation,  the  Office  of  Naval  Re- 
search, and  the  Public  Health  Service.  The  author  wishes  to 
thank  Dr.  Ruth  Hubbard  for  help  with  the  preparation  of 
this  manuscript. 


.sensitive  pigments  must  be  continuously  restored, 
or  vision  would  cease  soon  after  a  light  went  on. 
The  excitatory  state  must  also  be  rapidly  removed, 
or  vision  would  continue  long  after  a  light  went  off. 
It  would  aid  the  economy  of  such  a  system  if  these  re- 
actions were  coupled  so  as  to  complete  a  cycle  but 
this,  though  an  advantage,  is  not  essential.  All 
photoreceptor  processes  may  therefore  be  formulated 
as  follows: 


» Photosensitive  pigment 


-Excitatory  product^ 


light 


This  is  not  only  the  basic  arrangement  for  photo- 
reception  but,  generalized  to  include  stimuli  other 
than  light,  it  must  also  be  the  form  of  all  neural 
excitation.  Every  irritable  tissue  must  contain  similar 
arrangements  for  reacting  with  the  stimulus,  for 
removing  its  effects  and  for  restoring  the  original 
system.  One  may  therefore  expect  to  meet  the  same 
fundamental  pattern  of  reactions  at  every  level  of  the 
\isual  pathway;  and  the  entire  process  of  visual 
excitation  from  rods  and  cones  to  cerebral  cortex 
may  be  conceived  as  a  chain  of  such  .systems.  The 
peculiar  importance  of  the  photoreceptor  systems 
rests,  therefore,  not  on  their  intrinsic  form  but  on 
their  unique  sensitivity  to  light  and  their  initial 
position  in  the  chain,  by  virtue  of  which  certain  of 
their  properties  are  imposed  on  the  entire  visual 
response. 

Four  visual  pigments  are  known :  rhodopsin  and 
porphyropsin  in  rods,  and  iodopsin  and  cyanopsin 
in  cones.  All  of  them  are  built  upon  a  common  plan. 
They  are  all  carotenoid-proteins — proteins  bearing 
carotenoid  chromophores  to  which  they  owe  their 
color  and  sensitivity  to  light.  The  rhodopsin  system 
will  be  described  in  some  detail  since  it  provides  the 


671 


672 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


model  for  all  the  others.  Once  this  system  is  under- 
stood, the  others  emerge  as  simple  variants  upon  a 
common  theme. 


Rhudo/isiii 

Franz  Boll  discovered  the  red  pigment  rhodopsin 
in  the  rods  of  frogs  in  1876  (5).  It  is  characteristic 
of  the  rods  of  marine  fishes  and  land  \ertebrates 
(62).  There  is  no  evidence  (hat  it  is  ever  found  in 
cones. 

Some  years  ago  rhodopsin  was  shown  to  par- 
ticipate in  a  cycle  of  the  following  skeletal  form  (56) : 


Rhodopsin 


/■ 


Vitamin  A  +  opsin  < 
('visual  white') 


\Ught 
\ 

(orange  intermediates) 

\ 

Retinene  +  opsin 

('visual  yellow') 


Rhodopsin  bleaches  in  the  light  through  orange 
intermediates  to  a  mixture  of  the  yellow  carotenoid, 
retinene,  and  the  colorless  protein,  opsin  (fig.  i). 
The  retinene  is  then  converted  to  colorless  vitamin 
A.  Rhodopsin  is  resynthesized  on  the  one  hand  from 
retinene  and  opsin,  on  the  other  from  vitamin  A 
and  opsin. 

Morton  has  shown  that  retinene  is  vitamin  A 
aldehvde  (3): 


CH, 


CH;, 


C  CH,  CH,, 

y  \      hh|hhh|hh 
HjC  C— C=C— C=C— 0=C— C=C— C— OH 

I  II  H 

\  / 

c 

Vitamin  A 


FIG.  I.  .Absorption spectra  of  bullfrog 
rhodopsin  and  of  the  product  of  its 
bleaching  in  aqueous  digitonin  solution, 
pH  5.55-  Rhodopsin  possesses  three  ab- 
sorption maxima:  the  a-band,  mainly 
responsible  for  the  spectral  sensitivity  of 
rod  vision;  the  ^-band,  which,  like  a-, 
belongs  to  the  prosthetic  group;  and 
the  7-band,  due  to  the  protein  opsin. 
On  bleaching,  the  a-  and  (3-bands  are 
replaced  by  the  retinene  band  at  about 
385  m/i;  the  opsin  band  remains  un- 
changed. [From  Wald  (63).] 


2  Z 


2.0 


1.8 


1.6 


14 


l.i 


1.0 


0.8  - 


0.6  - 


0.4  - 


0.2 


Rhodops in 

B ie  ached  product 


200 


400  SOO 


600 


THE    PHOTORECEPTUR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


673 


CH, 


CH, 


B.C. 


H,C 


CH,  CH;, 

H    H     j      H    H    H     I      H    H 
-C=C—  c:=c—  C=C—  C=C—  c=o 


C— CH3 


C 
H. 


Retincne 


The  retinene  formed  Ijv  the  bleaching  of  rhodopsin 
is  reduced  to  vitamin  A  by  the  enzyme,  alcohol 
dehydrogenase,  working  together  with  the  coenzyme, 
DPN.  This  process  is  readily  carried  out  in  free 
solution  (fig.  2).  It  involves  only  the  transfer  of 
hydrogen  from  reduced  DPN  to  the  aldehyde  group 
of  retinene,  reducing  it  to  the  alcohol  group  of 
vitamin  A  (4,  64,  75): 


CsHjjCHO  +  DPN— H  +  H+ 
retinene 


alcohol  dehydrogenase 


C,,H„CH,OH  +  DPN+ 
vitamin  A 


0.4  - 


0.3  - 

C 

o 

^0.2 


Uj 


07  - 


0  - 


1 1 1 1 

/  taminAj-* —  ref  i nene  j 


Frog 

apoemyme 
+  DPN-H 


300 


400 
Wavelength  - mjj 


FIG.  2.  The  reduction  of  retinene  to  vitamin  .\.  Retinene 
was  mixed  in  digitonin  solution  with  the  enzyme,  alcohol 
dehydrogenase,  extracted  from  frog  retinas,  and  with  reduced 
cozymase  (DPN-H).  A  control  mixture  was  also  prepared 
which  differed  only  in  that  the  enzyme  had  been  kept  at  ioo°C 
for  0.5  min.  Both  mixtures  were  incubated,  then  extracted 
with  hexane.  The  absorption  spectra  of  the  hexane  extracts 
are  shown.  The  control  mixture  (^solid  circles')  contains  unaltered 
retinene;  the  mixture  containing  active  enzyme  (_open  circles) 
shows  complete  conversion  to  vitamin  .\.  [From  VVald  (64).] 


DPN  introduces  a  second  \itamin  into  the  chem- 
istry of  vision.  Its  active  principle  is  nicotinamide, 
the  antipellagra  factor  of  the  \itamin  B  complex. 
In  the  retina  it  is  in  the  curious  position  of  helping 
to  regenerate  vitamin  A. 

This  completes  the  degradative  processes  in  vision. 
Rhodopsin  having  been  bleached  by  light  to  a  mix- 
ture of  retinene  and  opsin,  the  retinene  is  reduced  to 
vitamin  A.  The  problem  now  is  to  go  back.  Kiihne 
already  recognized  this  to  be  a  dual  problem  (38). 
He  described  a  resynthesis  of  rhodopsiti  from  yellow 
precursors  (anagenesis)  which  was  relatively  rapid 
and  occurred  not  only  in  the  intact  eye  but  in  the 
isolated  retina  and  even  slightly  in  solution.  In 
addition  there  occurred  a  relatively  slow  synthesis 
of  rhodopsin  from  colorless  precursors  (neogenesis) 
which  Kiihne  could  ob.serve  only  in  the  intact  eye 
and  which  seemed  to  require  the  cooperation  of  the 
pigment  epithelium.  These  two  processes  can  now 
be  identified  with  the  synthesis  of  rhodopsin  from 
retinene  and  opsin,  and  from  vitamin  A  and  opsin. 

The  synthesis  of  rhodopsin  from  retinene  and  opsin 
is  a  spontaneous  reaction.  It  requires  neither  an 
enzyme  nor,  as  do  most  syntheses,  an  external  source 
of  energy.  One  has  only  to  bring  a  mixture  of  these 
two  substances  into  the  dark  to  form  rhodopsin 
(67).  Like  all  spontaneous  reactions,  it  is  an  energy- 
yielding  process,  which  can  therefore  do  work.  The 
work  it  does  in  vision  is  to  force  the  oxidation  of 
vitamin  A.  The  equilibrium  between  vitamin  A 
and  retinene  lies  far  over  toward  the  side  of  re- 
duction— toward  vitamin  A.  In  the  dark,  however, 
opsin  "traps'  retinene,  removing  it  to  form  rhodopsin, 
so  displacing  the  equilibrium  in  the  oxidative  di- 
rection. The  basic  mechanism  of  rhodopsin  synthesis, 
therefore,  is  the  energy-demanding  oxidation  of 
vitamin  A  to  retinene,  coupled  with  the  energy- 
yielding  condensation  of  retinene  and  opsin  to  form 
rhodopsin  (33,  76). 

One  important  consequence  of  this  arrangement 
is  that  it  is  self-limiting.  Vitamin  A  is  oxidized  to 
retinene  only  as  long  as  opsin  is  available  to  trap 
the  latter.  Retinene  therefore  never  accumulates. 
When  all  the  opsin  in  the  visual  receptors  has  been 
converted  to  \isual  pigments,  the  oxidation  of  vitamin 
A  automatically  ceases. 

The  rhodopsin  system  in  more  detail  therefore 
has  the  form  shown  in  figure  3.  Rhodopsin  is  con- 
verted by  light  to  the  orange-red  intermediate, 
lumi-rhodopsin.  .\t  temperatures  above  —  20°C 
this  goes  on  to  form  meta-rhodopsin;  and  with  access 


674 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


T^hodopsin 


l,ghf 


Lumi-rhodopiin 

[.>-20°C. 

Me  ta  -  rhodops  in 


alcohof  dehydrogenase 
DPfJ-H 

Vitamin  A,  i-Opsin  -■ =^ Fietinene,  -h  Opsin 


w 


VtiarrtinAj    from 

pigment  epithelium 

and  circufaiion 


DVN-H 


FIG.   3.  Diagram  of  the  rhodopsin  system.  [From  Hubbard 
&  Wald  (33).] 


to  water,  meta-rhodopsin  yields  retinene  and  opsin 
(74).  The  retinene  is  then  reduced  to  vitamin  A. 
In  the  dark,  the  spontaneous  combination  of  reti- 
nene and  opsin  to  form  rhodopsin  promotes  the  o.xi- 
dation  of  vitamin  A  to  retinene.  This  process  is 
aided  Idv  the  influx  of  new  vitamin  A  from  the  pig- 
ment epithelium  which  obtains  it  from  the  blood 
circulation,  by  the  provision  of  DPN,  the  oxidant  of 
vitamin  A,  and  by  respiratory  enzymes,  which  keep 
DPN  oxidized.  All  these  factors  acting  in  concert 
sweep  the  system  back  toward  rhodopsin  (33). 

It  should  be  noted  that  light  enters  this  scheme 
directly  at  only  one  point,  the  conversion  of  rhodopsin 
to  lumi-rhodopsin.  The  other  reactions  follow  from 
this  initial  act  but  are  themselves  'dark'  reactions, 
i.e.  reactions  which  proceed  equally  well  in  light 
or  darkness. 

Judging  from  figure  3,  it  should  be  possible  to 
assemble  the  rhodopsin  system  by  mixing  four  sub- 
stances in  solution:  vitamin  A,  opsin,  alcohol  de- 
hydrogenase and  DPN.  The  system  has,  in  fact, 
been  put  together  using  highly  purified  vitamin  A, 
crystalline  alcohol  dehydrogenase  from  horse  livers 
and  DPN  from  yeast.  The  only  component  that 
needs  to  be  obtained  from  the  retina,  and  indeed 
from  the  outer  segments  of  the  rods,  is  opsin.  This 
mixture,  placed  in  the  dark,  forms  rhodopsin. 
Brought  into  the  light  it  bleaches,  and  replaced  in 
the  dark  it  synthesizes  more  rhodopsin.  It  thus  per- 
forms in  solution  all  the  reactions  of  the  rhodopsin 
system  (33). 

However,  in  making  up  this  mixture,  not  all 
vitamin  A  is  effective.  \'itamin  A,  like  other  carot- 
enoids,    exists   in    a    number   of  different    molecular 


shapes,  cis-trans  isomers  of  one  another  (47,  83,  84). 
A.\\-trans  vitamin  A  (fig.  4),  the  predominant  isomer  in 
liver  and  blood  (Wald,  G.  &  P.  S.  Brown,  unpub- 
lished observations),  is  ineffective  in  rhodopsin 
synthesis.  Rhodopsin  requires  for  its  formation  one 
of  the  cis  isomers  of  vitamin  A  (34). 

According  to  theory,  only  two  of  the  four  side- 
chain  double  bonds  of  vitamin  A  should  be  capable  of 
forming  stable  m-linkages,  those  marked  9  and  13 
in  figure  4.  At  the  other  double  bonds,  a  cis  linkage 
encounters  serious  steric  hindrance,  and  the  molecule 
must  be  twisted  out  of  coplanarity.  This  interferes 
with  resonance  and  should  consequently  lead  to  a 
lowered  stability  (42,  43).  Only  four  geometrical 
isomers  of  vitamin  A  or  retinene  were  therefore 
expected:  a\\-trans,  g-cis,  I'^-cis,  and  9, 12,-dicis  (fig.  4). 

Five  cis-trans  isomers  of  retinene,  however,  have 
been    identified    and    crystallized   (10,    32,    39,    48): 


CH, 


CH, 


ripC  4  f,  t-  L.  t-  *-  \ 


1         I 


CH, 


a//-fran3 
viiamin  fi 


^c-^'^X 


H,C  C  ^C  C 


c  c 


reiinene 


13  —CIS 

(neo  -  a) 


CH, 


'3  » 


CH, 


Hx/^^v^N^N- 


H,C  C-CH,  c^ 

1^,  I 


9 -as 
ftso-a) 


CHj      ^C 


CH, 


CH, 


H,C  ^c  c  c 

'1  I  t^  I 


H,C  C-CHj 

^C-^  CH, 


h'-'^^-h 


9,)3  -d^as 
(iso  -  b) 


CH,  C 

(4 


FIG.  4.  Unhindered  geometrical  isomers  of  vitamin  .-K.  Tiiis 
molecule  can  assume  the  eis  configuration  only  at  double 
bonds  9  and  1 3  without  encountering  serious  steric  hindrance. 
At  the  other  double  bonds,  groups  come  into  conflict,  and  the 
CIS  configuration  not  only  bends  but  twists  the  molecule. 
[Modified  from  Hubbard  &   Wald  (34).] 


the  al\-trans  isomer,  originally  prepared  by  Ball  et  al. 
(3);  the  three  unhindered  cis  isomers — neo-a  (i3-«.s-), 
iso-a  (g-m)  and  iso-A  ((),\j,-dicis);  and  a  hindered 
cis  isomer  of  the  type  which  had  been  deemed  im- 
probable on  theoretical  grounds  (fig.  5).  This  hin- 
dered cis  isomer,  neo-A  (ii-m),  is  the  precursor  of 
rhodopsin  (32,  39).^ 

The  synthesis  of  rhodopsin  proceeds  in  two  stages. 
First,  vitamin  A  is  o.xidized  to  retinene;  then  rctinene 
couples  with  opsin.  The  first  process  is  relatively 
indiflferent  to  isomeric  configuration.  It  is  the  coupling 
of    retinene    with    opsin    that    is    isomer-spccific. 

On  incubation  with  opsin  in  the  dark,  neo-6 
retinene  yields  rhodopsin,  indistinguishable  from 
that  extracted  from  the  dark-adapted  retina.  On 
similar  treatment,  iso-a  retinene  yields  a  very  similar, 
light-sensitive  pigment,  with  its  X„,,,s  displaced  about 
13  m/i  toward  shorter  wavelengths.  This  is  called 
iso-rhodopsin.  The  remaining  isomers  are  inacti\e 
(fig.  6). 

When  rhodopsin  is  bleached,  the  retinene  which 
emerges  is  a\\-trans.  This  must  be  isomerized  to  the 
active  isomer,  neo-6,  before  it  can  resynthesize 
rhodopsin.  A  cycle  of  cis-trans  isomerization  is  there- 
fore   an    intrinsic    part    of   the    rhodopsin    system. 

A  single  passage  through  this  cycle  is  shown  in 
figure  7.  On  the  left,  a  mixture  of  neo-A  retinene  and 
cattle  opsin  in  aqueous  solution  incubated  in  the 
dark  forms  rhodopsin.  On  the  right,  the  rhodopsin 
formed  in  this  way  is  bleached  to  a  mixture  of  all- 
Irans  retinene  and  opsin.  The  extinction  of  retinene 
which  emerges  on  the  right  is  much  higher  than  that 
which  enters  on  the  left.  That  is  because  the  specific 
extinction  of  a\\-trans  retinene  is  higher  than  that  of 
the  neo-i  isomer. 

The  mechanism  by  which  the  eve  converts  all- 
trans  retinene,  which  results  from  bleaching  rho- 
dopsin, to  nco-h  retinene  is  not  entirely  clear.  All- 
trans  retinene  is  isomerized  to  a  mixture  of  cis  and 
tram  isomers  by  simple  exposure  to  light.  This  is  a 
second  photochemical  process  in  the  rhodopsin 
system.  The  eye  tissues  also  contain  an  enzyme, 
retinene  isomerase,  which  catalyzes  specifically  the 
interconversion  of  a\\-trans  and  neo-6  retinene,  and 
which  is  also  light-sensitive  (31).  There  probably  are 
additional  mechanisms  for  converting  aW-trans  rcti- 
nene or  vitamin  A  to  the  neo-A  isomer. 

The  rhodopsin  system  can  therefore  be  formulated 

^A  si.xth  isomer  of  retinene,  called  neo-c  (11,  lydicis"), 
has  since  been  synthesized  by  Oroshnik  (39). 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION  675 

as  follows  (34) : 

Rhodopsin 


Neo-6  retinene  -t-  opsin  - 


-.  W\-lrans  retinene  -|-  opsin 


[        (alcohol  dehydrogenase,  DPN) 


J 


Neo-A  vitamin  A  - 


;  All-;raHf  vitamin  A 


Vitamin  A  emerges  from  the  bleaching  of  rhodopsin 
as  the  free  alcohol;  yet  the  great  bulk  of  the  vitamin 


H,C  C 


^^'\./ 


C-CH3 

CH. 


CHf 


-% 


^c'"" 


.'T.'^'^^'H 


CH 


CHg^OH 


11 -cis    (neo-b) 


FIG.  5.  The  sterically  hindered  neo-b  (11 -aV)  isomer  of 
vitamin  A,  precursor  of  rhodopsin  and  iodopsin.  [From  Orosh- 
nik e/  al.  (40).] 


0.3  __ 


O.Z. 


0.1 


0._ 


-0./-_ 


"1 1 1 \ 

opstn  -f-  retinene  isomers 


o  a!I~iran5 
•  neoretinene  a 
9  neoretinGne  b 
e  isoreiinene  a 


—  o 

C 


wave  length  —  m/^ 


400 


450  500 


550 


600 


FIG.  6.  The  products  of  incubating  various  geometrical 
isomers  of  retinene  with  cattle  opsin.  Difference  spectra  are 
shown — differences  in  the  absorption  spectra  before  and  after 
bleaching  in  the  presence  of  hydroxylamine.  A\l-lrans  and 
neo-a  retinene  yield  no  light-sensitive  pigment.  Neo-A  retinene 
yields  rhodopsin;  iso-a  retinene,  iso-rhodopsin.  Iso-A  retinene, 
though  itself  inactive,  isomerizes  preferentially  to  iso-a  which 
yields  iso-rhodopsin.  [From  Hubbard  &  Wald  (34).] 


676 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  7.  Synthesis  and  bleaching  of 
rhodopsin  in  solution  (22.5°C,  pH  7.0). 
Left:  A  mixture  of  neo-A  retinene  and 
cattle  opsin  was  incubated  in  the  dark, 
and  absorption  spectra  recorded  peri- 
odically, (/)  at  0.3  min.,  (5)  at  2.5, 
(3)  at  5,  (^)  at  10,  (5)  at  18,  (ff)  at  30, 
(7)  at  60,  (<9)  at  120  and  (5)  at  180  min. 
The  absorption  band  of  neo-A  retinene 
(Xmax  380  m^i)  falls  regularly,  while  that 
of  rhodopsin  (X„,ax  498  m^)  rises.  Righl . 
The  rhodopsin  formed  at  the  left  (;)  is 
exposed  to  light  of  wavelengths  >550 
mil  for  various  intervals,  and  the  spec- 
trum is  recorded  immediately  after  each 
exposure.  The  total  irradiations  are:  (2) 

5  sec,  (3)  10  sec,  (^)  15  sec,  (5)  30 
sec,  and  (5)  120  sec.  The  residue  was 
exposed  for  45  sec.  longer  to  light  of 
wavelengths  >440  mn  (7).  [From  VVald 

6  Brown  (70).] 


300      400       SOO      600  400       500 

Wavelength- m/u 


600 


A  stored  in  the  eye  (primarily  in  the  pigmented 
layers,  choroid  and  pigment  epithelium)  is  in  the 
form  of  an  ester.  Recently  a  cell-free  enzyme  system 
has  been  prepared  from  cattle  retinas  and  pigmented 
layers  which  esterifies  vitamin  A  in  vitro  (38).  The 
significance  of  this  reaction  for  the  visual  cycle  is 
still  obscure.  It  is,  however,  noteworthy  that  the 
amount  of  vitamin  A  ester  stored  in  the  dark-adapted 
eye  is  roughly  equivalent  to  its  rhodopsin  content  on  a 
molar  basis,  and  that  the  neo-h  isomer  constitutes 
about  one-half  of  this  store  (37;  VVald,  G.  &  P.  S. 
Brown,  unpublished  observations). 

Porphyropsin 

The  rods  of  vertebrates  which  li\e  in,  or  better, 
spawn  in  fresh  water — fresh-water  fishes,  spaw^ning 
lampreys,  and  certain  larval  and  adult  amphibia — 
characteristically  contain  in  place  of  rhodopsin  a 
purple  light-sensitive  pigment  called  porphyropsin 
(62).  Its  X„i;,x  in  aqueous  .solution  ordinarily  is  close 
to  522  niM  (fig.  8).  On  bleaching,  it  yields  a  mixture 


of  opsin  and  a  new  retinene  called  retinenca,  and 
this  in  turn  is  reduced  to  a  new  vitamin  A  called 
\itamin  A-j.  It  was  the  analysis  of  this  visual  system 
that  led  to  the  discovery  of  these  carotenoids  (57, 
60).  The  structures  of  these  substances  have  now  been 
established  by  total  synthesis  (13).  They  differ  from 
\itamin  A  and  retinene' — sometimes  called  vitamin 
Ai  and  retinenei — only  in  possessing  an  additional 
double  bond  in  the  ring: 


CH3 


CH3 


C                        CH.,                    CH3 

/\     hh|hhh|hh 
H2C         c— c=c— c=c— c=c— c=c— c- 

1              II                                                         H 

-OH 

HC             C— CH3 

\    / 

c 

H 

Vitamin  A2 

2  Throughout  this  discussion,  the  terms  vitamin  A  and 
retinene  will  be  used  synonymously  with  vitamin  Ai  and 
retinenei. 


THE    PHOrORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


677 


CH, 


H,C 


HC 


\ 


C 
H 


CH3 

/ 

CH,  CH;, 

^        H    H     I      H     H    H     1      H    H 
C— C=C— C=C— C=C— c:=c— c=o 

II 
C-CH3 


Retinene., 


The  properties  of  the  porphyropsin  system  arc  in 
general  precisely  parallel  with  those  of  the  rhodopsin 
system.  Alcohol  dehydrogenase  and  DP\  catalyze 
the   equilibrium   between   retinenes  and   vitamin  Ao 


just  as  between  retinenei  and  vitamin  Ai  (64).  The 
bleaching  of  porphyropsin  yields  an  inactive  form  of 
retineneo,  apparently  the  dW-lrans  isomer. 

The  geometrical  isomers  of  retinenez  have  not  been 
investigated  as  thoroughly  as  those  of  retinenei. 
Nii-trans  retinene2  has  been  crystallized.  Two  cis 
forms  of  retinene2  have  been  partially  purified,  though 
not  crystallized.  These  resemble  in  their  spectro- 
scopic properties  respectively  the  neo-i  and  iso-o 
isomers  of  retinenei.  Neo-fe  retinene^,  when  incubated 
in  the  dark  with  opsin,  yields  porphyropsin,  in- 
distinguishable from  that  extracted  from  a  dark- 
adapted  fresh-water  fish  retina;  whereas  iso-a  retinene 
treated  similarly  yields  a  comparable  pigment,  iso- 


3.0 


2.5  - 


2.0  Y 

C 
.0 

0 

K 


1.0 


OS 


0  - 


1 \ — r 


"Porphyropsin  -  yo//ow/  perch 
•    unbleached 
o    bleached 


J L 


J 1 1 1 ^_ 


\ L 


300 


400 


£00 


600 


FIG.  8.  Absorption  spectra  of  porphyropsin  and  of  the  product  of  its  bleaching  (pH  7.0)  from 
the  fresh-water  yellow  perch,  Perca  flavescens.  This  preparation  was  extracted  with  2  per  cent  digi- 
tonin  from  a  suspension  of  rod  outer  segments,  which  had  been  previously  hardened  with  alum, 
and  pre-e.\tracted  with  water  and  with  petroleum  ether.  Porphyropsin,  like  rhodopsin,  possesses 
three  absorption  bands:  the  a-band  about  522  m^  at,  the  /3-band  at  about  377  m/x,  and  the  7-band 
(opsin)  at  about  280  m/i.  On  bleaching,  the  a-  and  /J-bands  are  replaced  by  the  absorption  band 
of  retineneo  ,  at  about  400  m^i.  [From  Wald,  G.,  P  K.  Brown  &  P.  .S.  Brown,  unpublished  ob- 
servations, i 


678 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEURiil'in^iOLGGY    I 


008\-  K 


T — r 


T 


Lepomia   op  sin 

o*  n*o-b    rei inQna^ 

•  ■mo- ex    rei tnanOi 


J \ 


be  formulated : 


FIG.  9  Synthesis  of  poiphyropsin  and  iso-porphyropsin.  The 
neo-A  and  iso-a  isomers  of  retinene;  ,  partially  purified,  were 
incubated  with  opsin  from  the  sunfish,  Lcpomis.  The  difference 
spectra  of  the  products  are  shown,  measured  in  the  presence 
of  hydroxylamine.  The  neo-6  isomer  yields  porphyropsin 
(Xmai  ,  522  m^i),  the  iso-a  isomer  iso-porphyropsin  (Xmax  , 
507  m^)-  [From  Wald,  G.,  P.  K.  Brown  &  P.  S.  Brown,  un- 
published observations.] 


porphyropsin,  with  X^ax  507  m^  (hg-  9;  Wald,  G., 
P.  K.  Brown  &  P.  S.  Brown,  unpublished  obser- 
vations). 

In  performing  such  syntheses  it  makes  no  difference 
whether  the  opsin  is  derived  from  a  fresh-water 
fish,  a  frog  or  cattle.  All  these  opsins  when  mixed 
with  neo-6  retinenej  yield  porphyropsin,  while  with 
neo-/>  retinenei  they  form  rhodopsin.  The  pigments 
obtained  with  cattle  opsin  lie  at  slightly  shorter 
wavelengths  than  those  obtained  with  frog  opsin: 
'cattle  porphyropsin'  lies  at  X,,,.,^  517  ni/j,  while 
'frog  porphyropsin'  has  Xmax  5-;-^  ni/i.  Cattle  and  frog 
rhodopsins  display  similar  differences:  the  former 
has  Xmax  498  m/i,  the  latter  Xmax  502  m^i.  Clearly 
species  differences  in  the  opsin  affect  the  X,„:,x  of 
the  visual  pigments. 

The  opsins  of  the  rods  that  have  been  examined  are 
so  closely  related  that  they  must  be  regarded  as 
belonging  to  the  same  family,  the  rod  opsins  or 
scotopsins.  The  rhodopsin  and  porphyropsin  systems 
therefore  share  entirely  the  same  proteins.  Only  their 
carotenoids  differ  and  those  only  by  one  double  bond 
in  the  ring.  The  porphyropsin  system  can  therefore 


Porphyropsin 


Neo-A  retinenco  -|-  scotopsin .KW-Uans  retinene-j  -\-  scotopsin 


[        (alcohol  dehydrogenase,  DPN) 


Neo-A  vitamin  Ao 


-^  All-(ra«i  vitamin  Aj 


lodopsin 

The  first  light-sensitive  pigment  of  cone  vision  was 
extracted  from  the  chicken  retina  in  1937.  It  is  a  violet 
pigment  (Xmax  562  m/x)  called  iodopsin.  The  chicken 
retina  contains  a  few  rods  among  a  large  predomi- 
nance of  cones  and  hence  yields  a  mixture  of  iodopsin 
and  rhodopsin  (58). 

The  carotenoids  of  the  iodopsin  system  are  identical 
with  those  of  the  rhodopsin  system,  even  to  cis-trans 
configuration.  Only  the  opsin  is  different.  The  cone 
opsins  can  be  called  photopsins.  The  replacement  of 
.scotopsin  by  photopsin  changes  the  rhodopsin  to 
the  iodopsin  system  (72) : 

lodopsin 

/  light 

/  \ 

..\\\-lrans  retinenei  -f  photopsin 


Neo-A  retinenei  +  photopsin 


(alcohol  dehydrogenase,  DPN) 


Neo-A  vitamin  Ai  — 


"  All-trans  vitamin  .^i 


From  the  light-adapted  chicken  retina  one  can 
extract  a  colorless  carotenoid-free  mixture  of  the 
proteins  of  rod  and  cone  vision,  scotopsin  and  pho- 
topsin. On  incubating  this,  or  a  wholly  bleached 
extract  of  chicken  retinas,  in  the  dark  with  neo-A 
retinenei,  one  obtains  a  mixture  of  rhodopsin  and 
iodopsin  indistinguishable  from  that  extracted  from 
the  dark-adapted  chicken  retina  (fig.  10). 

Just  as  iso-a  retinenei  yields  iso-rhodopsin  when 
incuijated  with  rod  opsin,  it  yields  a  similarly  dis- 
placed pigment,  iso-iodopsin,  on  incubation  with 
cone  opsin.  The  Xm.ix  of  iso-iodopsin  is  at  about 
515  m/i.  The  remaining  isomers  of  retinene  are  in- 
active (fig.  1 1). 

Cya7}opsin 

Rod  opsin  combines  with  nto-b  retinenei  to  yield 
rhodopsin,    or    with    neo-6    retineneo    to    yield    por- 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


679 


0.4 

-1     1 

— 1 r-    I    ■  I      1      1      1       1       1-1- 

Synthes/\s  of phoiopigmGnts 

~ 

from  chicHen  opsins-^ 

03 

r^^^                             •-*-  ca/cLf/ated  amount  re^/nene   _ 

4/ 

U 

c 

/o            ftv               •   ° "''  ^"^055  retinene 

0.d 

k!i 

/     \        \ 

V 

0.1 

- 

\          \ 

- 

X^^^  \. 

0 

-I L 

1        1        1        1        I.  -J \ \ \ L 

500 


700 


FIG.  10.  Successive  syntheses  of  iodopsin  and  rhodopsin 
in  solution.  An  extract  of  chicken  retinas  was  wholly  bleached 
with  an  orange  nonisomerizing  light  to  a  mixture  of  all-/ra«j 
retinene  and  rod  and  cone  opsins.  To  this  mixture  just  enough 
neo-A  retinene  was  added  to  regenerate  iodopsin  alone.  This 
amount  had  been  determined  by  preliminary  trial.  Iodopsin 
forms  so  much  more  rapidly  than  rhodopsin  that  its  synthesis 
is  complete  when  that  of  rhodopsin  has  scarcely  begun  (cf. 
fig.  2i).  The  absorption  spectrum  of  the  product,  formed  within 
a  few  minutes  in  the  dark,  is  shown  with  ^olid  circles.  Then  a 
small  excess  of  neo-b  retinene  was  added,  and  the  mixture  was 
reincubated  in  the  dark  for  30  min.  This  yielded  rhodopsin 
(_open  circles).  [From  VVald  el  al.  (72).] 


phyropsin.  Cone  opsin  combines  with  neo-^  retinenci 
to  yield  iodopsin.  Clearly  a  fourth  combination  is 
possible:    cone    opsin    with    neo-ft    retinenej. 

This  synthesis  was  recently  performed  in  our  labora- 
tory. It  yielded  a  blue  photosensitive  pigment  called 
cyanopsin  which  absorbs  maximally  in  the  orange- 
red,  at  about  620  m/z  (7i)-  Always  heretofore  knowl- 
edge of  a  visual  pigment  had  developed  in  the 
sequence:  recognition,  extraction,  analysis,  synthesis. 
With  cyanopsin  this  history  was  reversed.  A  pigment 
was  synthesized  in  solution  which  had  never  been 
identified   in  a  retina.    Had   it  a   place   in  vision? 

Where  would  one  look?  Obviously  in  retinas  which 
provide  its  ingredients:  cones,  hence  photopsin;  and 
vitamin  Ao.  One  might  therefore  look  for  cyanopsin 
in  a  fresh-water  fish  possessing  cones,  or  in  the  all- 
cone  retina  of  such  a  turtle  as  Pseudemys,  which  had 
been  shown  to  contain  \itamin  A2  CvO- 

Some  years  ago  Granit  measured  electrophysiologi- 
cally  the  spectral  sensitivity  of  cone  vision  in  a  fresh- 
water fish,  the  tench,  and  in  the  European  tortoise, 
Testudo  graeca  (15,  16).  His  measurements  are  shown 
as  the  points  in  figure  12;  the  line  is  the  main  ab- 
sorption   band    of  cyanopsin.    There   is   little   doubt 


that  cyanopsin  is  the  pigment  of  cone  vision  in  these 
animals. 

Recapitulation 

This  phase  of  the  chemistry  of  visual  excitation 
ends  on  a  very  simple  note.  The  visual  systems  which 
have  been  studied  involve  the  interaction  of  four 
substances:  a  rod  or  cone  opsin;  the  enzyme,  alcohol 
dehydrogenase;  the  coenzyme,  cozyniase;  and  neo-6 
Qii-cis)  vitamin  Ai  or  Aj.  They  can  be  summarized: 

light 


DPN+  I  -\-  rod  opsin rhodopsin 

vitamin  A,  .  retinene,    ,  'ight 

DNP-H  [-f  cone  opsin  .    iodopsin 

(alcohol  dehydrogenase) 

light 

DPN"*"  [  +rod  opsin .  porphyropsin 

vitamin  A2  retinene2  j  light 

^  cyanopsin 


DPN-H 


[-\-  cone  opsm  - 


In  addition  there  are  the  four  iso-pigments,  the 
carotenoid  chromophores  of  which  are  stereoiso- 
meric  with  those  of  the  visual  pigments.  Since  none 
of  the  iso-pigments  has  yet  been  found  in  a  retina, 
they  must  for  the  present  be  regarded  as  artifacts. 
How  does  the  retina  avoid  forming  them?  Prelimi- 
nary measurements  indicate  that  traces  of  iso-a 
vitamin  A  are  present  in  liver  oils,  while  in  cattle 
blood  the  iso-a  isomer  accounts  for  about  6  per  cent 
of  the  total  vitamin  A.  No  iso-a  vitamin  A  has  been 
detected  in  the  retina  and  pigment  layers  of  the  eye, 
whereas  the  neo-b  isomer  is  found  only  in  the  eye 
(Wald,  G.,  P.  K.  Brown  &  P.  S,  Brown,  unpublished 
observations).  It  is  therefore  likely  that  the  eye  actively 
forms  neo-6  vitamin  A — presumably  from  the  all- 
trans  isomer — and  actively  excludes  iso-a  vitamin  A. 

Role  of  Opsin  ill  I'isuai  Exeilation 

To  this  point  the  visual  pigments  have  been  dis- 
cussed mainly  from  the  point  of  view  of  their  carot- 
enoid components.  Their  properties,  however,  depend 
greatly  also  upon  the  opsins.  Though  their  color  and 
sensitivity  to  light  are  mediated  principally  through 
the  carotenoid  prosthetic  groups,  almost  everything 
else  derives  from  their  character  as  proteins.  Light 
liberates  retinene.  Yet,  like  other  carotenoids, 
retinene  is  a  bland,  relatively  inert  substance,  hardly 
capable  of  initiating  a  nervous  excitation.  Further- 
more, at  physiological  temperatures  and  pH  it  is 
released  relatively  slowly  as  the  last  step  in  a  chain 


680  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^   NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


T 


0.30  - 


0.20 


c 

o 

o  ._ 

c 


>< 


1 r 


T 


neor&iinene  a 


o //-trans  reiinene 


S'^nthes/s  of  iodops/ns 
from  sfGreo/somers 
of  retinenQ 


/so-  lodops/  n 

/ 


/oolops/n 


^ '•  ^'         necreiinene   h 


300 


400  SOO 

VMcxvelencjth-^  mjj 


600 


700 


FIG.  II.  Synthesis  of  iodopsin  and  iso-iodopsin.  In  a  chicken  retinal  extract,  the  iodopsin  alone 
was  bleached  with  deep  red  light  to  a  mi.xture  of  all-(ra;?.s  retincne  and  photopsin.  This  product 
was  incubated  in  the  dark  with  four  geometrical  isomers  of  retinene.  The  absorption  spectra  were 
then  measured  against  the  red-bleached  solution  as  blank.  .\\\-trans  and  neo-a  retinene  synthesized 
no  photosensitive  pigments,  hence  remained  almost  as  added.  Neo-i  retinene  formed  iodopsin 
C^inax  562  m^);  iso-a  retinene,  iso-iodopsin  (Xmax  510  m/i)-  Both  photosensitive  pigments  are  ac- 
companied by  residues  of  unchanged  retinene,  primarily  responsible  for  the  absorption  bands  at 
about  370  my..  [From  Wald  et  al.  (72).] 


of  reactions  initiated  by  light  (viz  fig.  3  and  text 
above),  whereas  the  nervous  response,  e\en  in  a 
cold-blooded  animal,  appears  within  a  fraction  of  a 
second.  Changes  in  the  opsins  therefore  would  seem 
to  offer  richer  possibilities. 

Rhodopsin  has  been  studied  most  in  this  regard. 
Cattle  rhodopsin  has  a  molecular  weight  of  about 
40,000  and  contains  one  molecule  of  retinene  (30). 
It  has  a  molar  extinction  of  40,600  (69).  The  iso- 
electric point  of  frog  rhodopsin  is  at  pH  4.47  and 
goes  to  pH  4.57  on  bleaching  (7);  cattle  rhodopsin 
is  isoionic  at  pH  5.4  and  goes  to  pH  5.5  on  bleaching 
(45).  Neither  cattle  rhodopsin  nor  opsin  contains 
available    N-    or    C-terminal    amino    acids    (i). 

The  synthesis  of  rhodopsin  from  retinene  and 
opsin  requires  the  presence  of  free  sulfhydryl  ( — SH) 
groups  on  opsin.   Conversely,   the  bleaching  of  rho- 


dopsin liberates  2  or  3  — SH  groups  per  molecule. 
This  is  true  equally  for  rhodopsins  from  cattle,  frogs 
and  squid  (68,  69).  Exposure  of  rhodopsin  to  light 
also  immediately  exposes  an  acid-binding  group  with  a 
pK  of  about  6.6,  close  to  the  pK  of  the  imidazole 
group  of  histidine  (45).  Furthermore,  opsin  is  much 
more  readily  denatured  by  acid  and  alkali,  or  heat, 
than  rhodopsin  (31a,  46). 

All  of  this  means  that  the  action  of  light  on  rho- 
dopsin, in  addition  to  splitting  off  carotenoid,  pro- 
foundly affects  the  reactivity  of  the  opsin.  In  the 
structural  context  of  a  rod  outer  limb,  these  or  like 
changes  are  probabh'  the  ultimate  source  of  excita- 
tion. 

It  is  important  to  realize  that  rhodopsin  is  one  of 
the  principal  structural  components  of  a  rod.  It 
accounts  for  about  40  per  cent  of  the  dry  weight  of 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


68 1 


7^ 


W 


to 

C   OS 

(1) 

<o 

o 


06    - 


0/S 


C   0.2 

0 


•  Cyanopsm   absorption 

Spectral  sensiiivity. 
<D   'tortoise 
©   trench 


V^ave1enc)th-m)j 

I        I        I        I ^ 


soo 


600 


700 


FIG.  12.  The  absorption  spectrum  of  cyanopsin  compared  with  Granit's  electrophysiological 
measurements  of  the  spectral  sensitivity  of  cone  vision  in  a  fresh-water  fish,  the  tench,  and  in  the 
European  tortoise,   Testudo  graeca.  [From  Wald  et  a/.  (71).] 


the  outer  segment  of  a  frog  rod,  or  about  60  per  cent 
of  the  nonlipid  dry  weight.  In  cattle  rods,  it  accounts 
for  about  14  per  cent  of  the  dry  weight  of  the  outer 
segment,  or  about  22  per  cent  of  the  nonlipid  dry 
weight  (30).  The  outer  segments  of  the  rods  and 
cones  are  layered  structures  composed  of  several 
hundred  to  several  thousand  layers,  apparently  of 
protein,  each  about  40  to  160  A  thick  (52).  The 
membranes  of  the  rod  must  be  made  in  large  part  of 
rhodopsin  (or  porphyropsin).  A  cone  has  much  the 
same  construction,  though  in  some  cones  the  visual 
pigments  may  compose  a  smaller  fraction  of  the 
membranes  (65). 

Two  model  systems  have  been  described  in  which 
the  bleaching  of  rhodopsin  in  solution  registers 
directly  as  an  electrical  fluctuation  (45,  68).  Both  are 
based  on  the  fact  that  light  exposes  ion-binding 
groups  on  opsin,  sulfhydryl  groups  in  one  case,  an 
acid-binding  group  with  pK  6.6  in  the  other,  which 
aflTect  the  ion  concentration  in  the  medium.  These 
models  show  that  rhodopsin  has  the  capacity  to 
translate  the  absorption  of  a  quantum  of  light  into 
an  electrical  event.  The  eflfective  utilization  of  this 
capacity  depends  entirely  upon  the  structural  frame- 
work within  which  it  occurs.  A  dark-adapted  rod  is 
stimulated  by  the  absorption  of  a  single  quantum  of 
light  (6,  29,  44).  The  same  probably  is  true  of  a 
dark-adapted  cone.  One  quantum  of  light  is  absorbed 


by  one  molecule  of  visual  pigment,  and  a  rod  or  cone 
is  so  peculiarly  constructed  that  so  small  a  change 
can  excite  it. 


PHYSIOLOGICAL    CORREL.ATIONS 

Every  physiological  function,  normal  and  path- 
ological, has  its  roots  in  biochemistry;  conversely 
every  facet  of  biochemistry  finds  expression  in  the 
properties  and  behavior  of  the  organism.  In  a  sense 
the  organism  is  a  macroscopic  representation  of 
certain  of  its  component  molecules,  and  one  of  the 
principal  tasks  of  physiology  is  to  learn  to  read  its 
features  in  their  features. 

This  is  nowhere  plainer  than  in  \ision.  The  re- 
actions initiated  by  light  in  the  rods  and  cones  in- 
troduce a  long  train  of  nervous  and  s\naptic  proc- 
esses which  end  in  visual  sensations.  The  primary 
events  have  been  described  in  some  detail.  The 
visual  apparatus  as  a  whole  is  largely  concerned 
with  conducting  the  information  they  dictate.  For 
this  reason  many  of  the  basic  properties  of  vision 
reflect  simply  and  directly  the  properties  of  retinal 
molecules. 

It  is  of  the  highest  importance  to  explore  lhe.se 
relationships.  Needless  to  say,  there  is  much  more  in 
vision  than  photochemistry,  or  indeed  than  any  of 


682 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


the  peripheral  processes  one  can  measure.  Yet  it  is 
important  to  learn  how  far  one  can  come  with  these 
if  only  to  know  that  one  must  seek  elsewhere  for 
what  remains. 

Absorption  Spectra  and  Spectral  Sensitivity: 
Purkinje  Phenomenon 

The  rise  and  fall  of  \isual  sensitivity  throughout 
the  spectrum  is  governed  in  the  first  instance  by  the 
capacity  of  the  visual  pigments  to  absorb  light  of 
various  wavelengths,  i.e.  by  their  absorption  spectra. 
When  properly  corrected,  the  spectral  sensitivity 
should  correspond  closely  with  the  absorption  spectra 
of  the  visual  pigments. 

For  such  a  comparison,  the  spectral  sensitivity 
must  be  corrected  for  distortions  caused  by  colored 
ocular  structures,  in  the  human  eye  principally  the 
yellow  lens  and  macula  lutea  and  similar  structures 
in  the  eyes  of  other  animals.  The  spectral  sensitivity 
also  should  be  quantized.  What  is  measured  generally 
is  the  relative  energy  at  each  wavelength  needed  to 
evoke  a  constant  response.  The  reciprocal  of  this  is 
the  relative  sensitivity,  and  this  divided  by  the  wave- 
length is  the  sensitivity  in  terms  of  relative  numbers 
of  incident  quanta.  This  is  the  form  in  which  spectral 
sensitivity  data  can  best  be  employed  for  the  present 
purpose. 

The  spectra  of  the  visual  pigments  should  be  stated 
in  terms  of  percentage  ab.sorption  rather  than  ex- 
tinction (cf  59).  The  point  of  this  distinction  is  that 
all  extinction  curves  are  simple  multiples  of  one 
another,  whereas  a  percentage  absorption  curve  has  a 
unique  shape  depending  upon  the  actual  value  of  the 
absorption.  However,  extinction  and  percentage 
absorption  are  almost  exactly  proportional  to  each 
other  up  to  10  per  cent  absorption  and  depart  only 
slightly  from  proportionality  up  to  aljout  20  per 
cent  absorption.  All  known  cones  and  most  rods  seem 
to  have  absorptions  below  this  value.  Extinction 
therefore  runs  parallel  with  absorption  for  all  cones 
and  for  all  but  the  more  densely  pigmented  rods. 
In  the  figures  which  follow,  the  absorption  spectra 
of  the  visual  pigments  have  been  plotted  in  terms 
of  relative  extinction  since  the  percentage  absorption 
usually  is  not  known.  This  introduces  appreciable 
distortion  only  in  comparison  with  frog  rod   \ision 

(cf.  fig.  14)- 

Figure  13  shows  the  comparison  between  the 
absorption  spectra  of  chicken  rhodopsin  and  iodopsin, 
and  the  spectral  sensitivity  of  rod  and  cone  vision  in 


the  pigeon.  It  would,  of  course,  be  preferable  to  com- 
pare the  spectral  sensitivity  of  the  chicken,  but 
in  the  absence  of  accurate  data  measurements  on  the 
closely  related  pigeon  have  been  used.  They  were 
obtained  by  inserting  microelectrodes  into  the  retina, 
following  removal  of  the  lens  and  cornea  (11,  19). 
The  pigeons  were  either  dark-adapted  i  to  2  hours 
following  the  operation,  or  were  light-adapted.  At 
each  wa\elength,  measurements  were  made  of 
the  energy  needed  to  evoke  a  constant  electrical 
response.  The  reciprocal  of  the  energy,  the  sensi- 
tivity, was  quantized  by  dividing  by  the  wavelength. 

The  scotopic  sensitivity  agrees  very  well  with  the 
absorption  spectrum  of  rhodopsin.  The  photopic 
sensitivity  however  is  displaced  about  20  m/i  toward 
the  red  from  the  spectrum  of  iodopsin.  This  displace- 
ment must  be  caused  in  large  part  by  the  brightly- 
colored  oil  globules  which  lie  in  the  cones  of  chickens 
and  pigeons  in  the  position  of  color  filters  (79,  80). 
The  displacement  seems  larger  than  the  color  filters 
of  the  chicken  retina  should  cause  and  may  mean 
that  many  of  the  electrophysiological  measurements 
happened  to  fall  within  the  'red  field'  of  the  pigeon 
retina,  the  dorsotemporal  quadrant  in  which  deep 
red  oil  globules  predominate. 

The  shift  of  spectral  sensitivity  toward  the  red  as 
one  goes  from  scotopic  to  photopic  conditions,  from 
rod  to  cone  vision,  is  the  well-known  Purkinje 
phenomenon.  Except  for  the  distortion  just  alluded 
to,  this  is  accurately  mimicked  in  solution  by  the 
absorption    spectra    of    rhodopsin    and    iodopsin. 

This  comparison  gains  special  force  when  made 
with  retinas  which  do  not  possess  obviously  colored 
filtering  pigments.  In  figure  14  the  absorption 
spectra  of  chicken  rhodopsin  and  iodopsin  are 
compared  with  the  spectral  sensitivities  of  rod  and 
cone  vision  in  the  frog,  snake,  guinea  pig  and  cat, 
measured  with  electrical  procedures  by  Granit  and 
co-workers.  The  scotopic  data  agree  very  well  with 
the  absorption  spectrum  of  rhodopsin.  The  photopic 
sensitivities  agree  so  well  with  the  absorption  spec- 
trum of  iodopsin  that  it  seems  probable  that  this  is 
the  major  pigment  of  cone  vision  in  the  frog,  snake 
and  cat. 

Figure  14  shows  that  when  colored  ocular  struc- 
tures do  not  intervene,  the  Purkinje  phenomenon 
emerges  quantitatively  from  the  absorption  spectra  of 
rhodopsin  and  iodopsin.  In  essence  it  in\olves 
nothing  more  than  the  transfer  of  vision  from  de- 
pendence on  the  absorption  spectrum  of  rhodopsin 
in  dim  light   to   that  of  iodopsin  in   bright  light. 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


683 


1.2 


1.0 


0.8  - 


0.6- 


9  0.4   - 


0.2 


O  - 


- 

1 — 1 — \ — 1 — \ —  I      I  ■- 

ChicKen  visual  picfmenis  vs. 

P'9^ 

-1 — \ — I      I 

on  retinaj  sens/ 

— ^ — \ r-." ' 

t/v/ty 

1 

- 

/ 

/ 

^-^ 

— 

- 

rhodopsin^^l 

/ 
/ 

e 

\ 

- 

- 

/  / 

V 

\ 

,         \^/odops/n 

- 

scoiopic     Yv 
sensitivity  J 

// 

\ 

\ 

\         sensitivity 

/    / 

\ 

\ 

- 

/7 
/   / 

•\ 

\ 

\ 

\ 

- 

- 

f^  *                        j/ 

/   / 

\ 

\\ 

- 

- 

^r 

y 

\ 

\ 

V 

- 

1 \ \ L_ 

Wavelength  - 

_J , 1 1 — 

-mjj 

^^ 

._.•__ 

XL 

-     I- 

'    ,      '^       ? 

400 


soo 


6O0 


700 


FIG.  13.  Absorption  spectra  of  chicken  rhodopsin  and  iodopsin,  compared  with  the  spectral 
sensitivities  of  dark-  and  Hght-adapted  pigeons.  The  latter  were  measured  electrophysiologically 
and  are  plotted  in  terms  of  the  reciprocals  of  the  numbers  of  incident  quanta  needed  to  evoke  a 
constant  electrical  response.  The  scotopic  data  are  from  Donner  (11),  the  photopic  data  from  the 
same  source  (^barred  circles')  and  from  Granit  (19)  (open  circles').  The  scotopic  sensitivity  agrees  well 
with  the  absorption  spectrum  of  rhodopsin.  The  photopic  sensitivity  is  displaced  about  20  m^ 
toward  the  red  from  the  absorption  spectrum  of  iodopsin,  owing  in  large  part  to  the  filtering  action 
of  the  colored  oil  globules  of  the  pigeon  cones.   [From  Wald  (72).] 


FIG.  14.  The  absorption  spectra  of  chicken  rhodopsin 
(Xmax  502  m/i)  and  iodopsin  (Xmax  562  my.)  compared  with  the 
scotopic  and  photopic  sensitivities  of  various  animals.  The 
lines  show  the  absorption  spectra  of  the  visual  pigments,  the 
points  electrophysiological  measurements  of  spectral  sensitivity 
(quantized).  Scotopic  data:  frog  (22);  cat  (12);  guinea  pig 
(18).  Photopic  measurements;  frog  (17);  snake  (20);  cat  (21). 
[From  Wald  et  al.  (72).] 


Figure  15  shows  this  same  comparison  for  the 
human  eye.  The  spectral  sensitivities  were  measured 
in  the  periphery  of  the  aphakic  (lensless)  eye,  to 
avoid  distortions  otherwise  introduced  by  the  yellow 
pigmentations  of  the  lens  and  macula  lutea  (61,  63). 
The  scotopic  sensitivity  agrees  well  with  the  ab- 
sorption spectrum  of  rhodopsin,  but  the  photopic 
sensitivity  is  displaced  about  20  m/x  toward  the  blue 
from  iodopsin.  This  is  hardly  surprising,  for  the 
human  photopic  sensitivity  is  believed  to  be  a  com- 
posite function,  the  resultant  of  the  spectral  sensitivi- 
ties of  at  least  three  classes  of  cone  needed  to  account 
tor  trichromatic  vision.  These  seem  to  possess  maxima 
at  about  450,  550  and  590  mix  (2,  53).  Iodopsin,  or  a 
clo.sely  related  pigment,  may  function  as  the  middle 
member  of  this  trio,  but  this  must  cooperate  with  at 
least  two  other  cone  pigments  to  provide  the  mech- 
anism of  normal  color  differentiation. 

Finally,  in  figure  16,  such  a  comparison  is  shown 
for  the  vitamin  A2  eye  of  a  fresh-water  fish,  the  tench. 
The    spectral    sensitivities,    scotopic    and    photopic, 


684 


HANDBOOK    OF    PinSlOI.OCV   -^    NEUROPHYSIOLOCJY    I 


\ \ 1 \ \ 

ChicHen  :    o  iodopsin 

•  rhodopsin 

Human  lensless  peripheral 
vision:    O  cones 

•  nods 


rhodopsin 
rod  vision 


1 — I — I — I — r 


1.2  - 


1.0 


p  0.8 
5 

§    0.6 
o 

u 

l5 


0.2- 


0- 


I       r 


iodopsin 
cone   vision 


400 


500 


600 


700 


FIG.  15.  Absorption  spectra  of  chicken  rliodopsin  and  iodopsin  compared  with  the  spectral 
sensitivity  of  human  rod  and  cone  vision.  The  spectral  sensitivity  measurements  were  made  in  a 
peripheral  field  in  the  aphakic  (lensless)  eye  to  avoid  distortions  caused  by  the  yellow  pigmenta- 
tions of  the  lens  and  macula  lutea.  They  represent  as  close  an  approximation  to  the  sensitivities 
of  the  naked  rods  and  cones  as  can  be  achieved  in  the  living  eye  (cf.  61,  63).  The  scotopic  (rod) 
sensitivity  agrees  with  the  absorption  spectrum  of  rhodopsin  over  most  of  its  course.  The  photopic 
(cone)  sensitivity  is  displaced  some  20  m^i  toward  the  blue  from  the  absorption  spectrum  of  iodopsin; 
it  represents  the  resultant  of  the  spectral  sensitivities  of  at  least  three  groups  of  cones  concerned 
with  color  vision.  [From  VVald  li  at.  (72).] 


measured  electrophysiologically,  are  shown  as  large 
circles.  The  lines  and  small  circles  show  the  ab- 
sorption spectra  of  porphyropsin  and  cyanopsin. 
The  photopic  sensitivity  agrees  very  well  with  the 
absorption  spectrum  of  cyanopsin;  but  for  reasons 
which  are  still  obscure,  the  scotopic  sensitivity  is 
displaced  about  10  m/i  toward  the  red  from  por- 
phvropsin.  The  corneas  and  lenses  had  been  remo\ed 
from  these  preparations;  possibly  some  yellow  pig- 
mentation in  the  retina  or  a  trace  of  blood  in  the 
ocular  fluids  may  account  for  this  discrepancy,^  In 
animals  having  vision   based   upon  \itamin  A..,   the 

'  Recently  it  has  been  shown  that  the  absorption  spectra  of 
visual  pigments  in  silu  lie  about  7  mix  toward  the  red  from  their 
positions  in  solution  (ga,  70a). 


Purkinje  shift  is  unusually  large:  about  90  m/z, 
from  about  530  m/x  in  the  scotopic  eye  to  about  620 
mix  in  the  photopic  eye.  This  is  consistent  with  the 
large  displacement  between  the  absorption  spectra 
of  porphyropsin  and  cyanopsin. 

It  can  be  concluded  that  the  spectral  sensitivities 
of  rod  and  cone  vision,  and  hence  the  Purkinje 
phenomenon,  derive  directly  and  quaiititati\ely 
from  the  absorption  spectra  of  the  \isual  pigments. 


Visual  Adaplalion  and  the  Bleaching  and 
Synthesis  of  Visual  Pigments 

It    has   been   believed   for   many   years   that   soine 
simple    relation    connects    the    visual    threshold,    or 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


68  n 


}.0  - 


^ 


t  oQ-      porphyropsin^ 


C 

c 
o 

■  ^04 


Q 


02- 


0 


1 \ \ 1 \ \ 1 \ \ \ \ \        \        \        I        \ 

T'orphyropsin,  cyanopsin,  and  spectral  sensitivity  of  the  tench 


scotopic 
-sensitivity 


cyanopsin 


photopic 
sensitivity 


A L 


_L 


_L 


A L 


SOO  600 

Wavelengrt/i  -  m/j 


700 


FIG.  1 6.  Absorption  spectra  of  porphyropsin  and  cyanopsin  Clines,  small  circles^  compared  with 
the  spectral  sensitivities  of  rod  and  cone  vision  in  a  fresh-water  fish,  the  tench  (broken  line,  large 
circles').  The  spectral  sensitivities  were  measured  electrophysiologically  by  Granit  (i6)  in  opened 
eyes  from  which  cornea  and  lens  had  been  removed.  The  photopic  sensitivity  agrees  well  with  the 
absorption  spectrum  of  cyanopsin,  but  the  scotopic  sensitivity  is  displaced  about  lo  mM  toward 
the  red  from  porphyropsin,  perhaps  because  of  some  yellow  pigmentation  in  the  retina  or  oculai" 
fluids. 


better  its  reciprocal,  the  visual  sensitivity,  with  the 
concentration  of  visual  pigment.  It  has  been  as- 
sumed that  in  a  steady  illumination  the  visual  pig- 
ments bleach  to  steady  levels,  maintained  thereafter 
by  regenerative  processes.  Simultaneously  the  visual 
sensitivity  falls  to  a  steady  state  value;  this  is  light 
adaptation.  Conversely,  in  the  dark  the  vi-sual  pig- 
ments are  synthesized  to  their  maximal  concentra- 
tions. Simultaneously  the  sensitivity  rises  to  a  maxi- 
mum; this  is  dark  adaptation. 

Lately  it  has  become  apparent  that  whatever 
relation  obtains  between  visual  sensitivity  and  con- 
centration of  visual  pigment  is  not  as  direct  as 
simple  proportionality.  On  the  contrary,  the  bleach- 
ing of  a  very  small  fraction  of  rhodopsin  in  dark- 
adapted  rods  results  in  an  extraordinarily  large  fall 
of  sensitivity  (51).  Parallel  'light  adaptations'  con- 
ducted on  a  human  subject  and  on  a  solution  of 
cattle  rhodopsin  in  a  water  model  of  the  human  eye 
show   that,   to  a  first  approximation,   the   bleaching 


of  0.006  per  cent  of  the  rhodopsin  lowers  the  visual 
sensitivity  8.5  times;  and  the  bleaching  of  0.6  per 
cent  of  rhodopsin  lowers  the  sensitivity  3300  times 
(65).  Conversely  the  resynthesis  of  the  last  small 
fraction  of  rhodopsin  must  raise  the  sensitivity 
greatly.  Indeed  much  of  light  and  dark  adaptation 
in  the  rods  seems  to  involve  the  first  small  fraction  of 
rhodopsin  to  be  bleached,  and  the  last  small  fraction 
to  be  resynthesized  (cf.  23,  24). 

Recently  Rushton  and  his  co-workers  have  suc- 
ceeded by  a  most  ingenious  procedure  in  measuring 
directly  the  rise  and  fall  of  visual  pigment  in  the 
living  human  eye  (8,  49,  50).  This  permits  a  direct 
comparison  between  the  rates  of  bleaching  and  syn- 
thesis of  photosensitive  pigments  and  the  course  ol 
light  and  dark  adaptation.  For  measuring  rhodopsin, 
the  method  depends  on  comparing  the  reflection 
from  the  retina  of  a  blue-green  light  strongly  ab- 
sorbed by  rhodopsin  with  an  orange  light  scarcely 
absorbed    by    rhodopsin.    No   change    of  retinal   re- 


686 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


flectance  was  detected  on  illuminating  such  rod-free 
areas  as  the  fovea  or  the  optic  disc.  On  light-adapting 
areas  known  to  contain  rods,  increases  in  the  re- 
flectance of  blue-green  light  were  recorded,  ap- 
parently caused  by  bleaching  rhodopsin.  The  vari- 
ation in  magnitude  of  this  effect  along  the  horizontal 
meridian,  from  nasal  to  temporal,  can  be  correlated 
with    the    distribution    of   rod    density    (fig.     17). 

When  the  eye  is  exposed  to  light,  the  rhodopsin 
content  falls  exponentially  to  a  steady  state  level  at 
which  the  rate  of  bleaching  is  balanced  by  the 
regeneration  rate.  As  might  be  expected,  the  rho- 
dopsin content  at  the  steady  state  decreases  as  the 
level  of  illumination  is  raised  (fig.  18).  The  time 
course  of  bleaching  roughly  parallels  the  course  of 
light  adaptation  of  human  rod  vision  (cf.  73).  Fol- 
lowing light  adaptation,  the  rhodopsin  concentra- 
tion rises  regularly  in  the  dark  (fig.  18)  and  ap- 
proaches a  maximum  value  in  about  thirty  minutes 
(50),  in  good  agreement  with  the  time  required  for 
human  rod  dark  adaptation  (fig.  19). 

The  course  of  bleaching  and  rcsynthesis  of  rhodopsin 
in  the  human  retina,  measured  in  this  way,  agrees 
with  the  course  of  human  light  and  dark  adaptation 
only  when  the  latter  is  plotted  in  terms  of  log  sen- 
sitivity. It  is  the  logarithm  of  the  visual  sensitivity 
that  rises  and  falls  with  time  much  as  does  the  con- 
centration of  rhodopsin.  A  theory  has  been  proposed 
which  accounts  for  this  relationship  (65,  72).  The 
rod  is  viewed  as  a  compartmented  structure.  Each 
compartment  contains  a  large  quantity  of  rhodopsin 
and  is  discharged  by  the  absorption  of  a  first  quantum 
of  light.    The    residual    rhodopsin    of   a    discharged 


160 


'■120 


80 


• 

• 

-     .. 

■      III 

■5 

s. 
0 

• 

\      r      °^\ 

• 
1        8*      1        1        1        1        1 

50 


-fO       30 
•Nasal 


10        0         10 
Degrees 


20        30        40 
—  Temporal 


FIG.  17.  Distribution  of  rhoclop.sin  den.sity  in  the  human 
retina.  Circles:  measurements  of  rhodopsin  density  at  tlie  points 
shown  along  the  horizontal  meridian.  Line:  rod  density  per 
mm''  in  the  same  region.  [From  Campbell  &  Rushton  (8).] 


i 


i 

.  1 

.x--^'^^ 

- 

V 

1       /^"^ 

' 

- 

/ 

\^ 

_ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

y  100 

1 

10 
Time  (mm) 


FIG.  18.  Bleaching  and  resynthesis  of  rhodopsin  in  the 
human  retina  15  degrees  temporal  to  the  fovea.  Open  circles: 
On  exposing  the  eye  successively  to  lights  of  increasing  bright- 
ness (i,  5  and  100  units,  where  i  unit  =  20,000  trolands), 
the  rhodopsin  content  falls  each  time  to  a  new  steady-state 
level  at  which  the  rate  of  bleaching  is  balanced  by  the  regenera 
tion  rate.  Filled  circles:  In  the  dark,  rhodopsin  regenerates. 
Complete  recovery  (not  shown  in  figure)  takes  about  30  min. 
(50).  [From  Campbell  &  Rushton  (8).] 


compartment  continues  to  absorb  light  and  to 
bleach  but  can  no  longer  contribute  to  excitation. 
A  rod  is  rendered  wholly  inexcitable  when  each  of 
its  compartments  has  absorbed  at  least  one  quantum 
of  light,  i.e.  when  in  each  of  its  compartments  at 
least  one  molecule  of  rhodopsin  has  been  bleached. 
In  this  way  the  bleaching  of  very  little  rhodopsin 
can  lead  to  a  high  state  of  light  adaptation^.  This 
hypothesis,  pursued  mathematically,  leads  to  the 
expectation  that  the  logarithm  of  the  visual  sensitivity 
should  be  appro.ximately  proportional  to  the  con- 
centration of  visual  pigment  (72). 

The  same  relationships  appear  to  hold  for  cones. 
Rushton  (49)  has  recently  modified  his  method  to 
measure  cone  pigments  in  the  human  fovea.  He 
finds  that  in  the  dark,  following  exposure  to  a  bright 
light,  cone  visual  pigment  is  resynthesized  much  more 
rapidly  than  rhodopsin  (fig.  20).  The  course  of 
synthesis  parallels  human  cone  dark  adaptation  (fig. 
19).  It  has  long  been  known  that  in  man  and  many 
other  animals  the  cones  dark-adapt  much  more 
rapidly  than  the  rods.   In  the  human  eye  the  dark 

^ The  term  bleach  is  here  used  loosely  to  in\oK'e  the  entire 
chain  of  eflfects  that  follows  the  absorption  of  light  by  rho- 
dopsin. The  first  such  effect  is  the  production  of  lumi-rhodopsin; 
then  by  thermal  reactions  meta-rhodopsin  (still  without  literal 
bleaching);  and  finally  a  mixture  of  all-/ran.f  retinene  and  opsin. 
The  excitation  process  probably  depends  upon  the  first  of 
these  steps,   the  change   to  lumi-  or  at  most   mcta-rhodopsln. 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION  687 


8 

—I 

-I — 

-r 

1 

-| 1 1 \ r      1       1       1       1 

-T"     I          I 

I          I 

Human  da 

'•k  actapia-iion  — 

-    >s 

/i" 

atiova  fovQtx 

_ 

0 

rit)h-i 

eye 

«-•«— 

7 

> 

• 

lafi  eye                                                              .  _2 ^^ 

—  •                  0 

- 

:♦; 

^.^n"""""'^'^ 

5 

^^f^t^"^*^ 

-  c 

Q^^^ 

'~ 

• 

J^ 

to 

•  X^ 

6 

-  « 
> 

• 

J^           rod^ 

- 

S 

0 

/ 

r 

^3- 

^  "    » 

/ 
/ 

/ 

- 

4 

/ 

_ 

7 

cono^ 

3 

J 

-J — 

i.. 

-+- 

T7m0  in  afarH  ^  m/nw^es 

-\ — i — \ — \ — \ — I — 1 — I — \ — 

J \ \ 

4 L- 

10 


ts 


20 


2S 


30 


3S 


40 


FIG.  19.  Dark-adaptation  of  the  human  eye  measured  in  a  peripheral  area  which  contains  both 
rods  and  cones.  The  dark  adaptation  of  the  cones  is  completed  within  about  5  min.,  that  of  the  rods 
within  about  45  min.  [From  VVald  et  al.  (72).] 


S  01 

V 

Q 


30  sec  bleachinq 

with  strong  orange  liqht 


Subject  J  WHD 


FIG.  20.  Bleaching  and  resynthesis  of  visual  pigments  in 
the  human  fovea.  Initial  values  after  dark  adaptation.  Fol- 
lowing a  30  sec.  bleach  with  strong  orange  light,  the  density 
is  at  first  very  low  but  rises  rapidly  in  the  dark.  Recoscry  is 
complete  in  6  min.  [From  Rushton  (49).] 


adaptation  of  the  cones  is  complete  within  4  to  6 
min.,  while  that  of  the  rods  continues  for  over  45 
min.  The  dark  adaptation  of  a  peripheral  area  of  the 
human  retina  containing  rods  and  cones  is  shown 
in  figure  19.  It  is  plotted  in  terms  of  log  sensitivity 
(-log  threshold)  the  better  to  expose  its  relationship 
to    the    rise    of   \isual    pie;ment    concentration. 


Another  approach  to  this  problem  has  been  made 
by  comparing  the  rates  of  synthesis  of  rhodopsin 
and  iodopsin  in  solution.  Figure  10  above  shows  a 
mixture  of  chicken  iodopsin  and  rhodopsin  made  by 
incubating  nco-b  retinene  in  solution  with  a  mixture 
of  cone  and  rod  opsins.  The  reason  the  visual  pig- 
ments form  separately  in  this  instance  is  that  iodopsin 
is  synthesized  with  enormously  greater  speed  than 
rhodopsin,  about  530  times  as  fast  at  io°C  (72). 
Figure  21  shows  the  synthesis  of  the  two  pigments  in 
solution  at  23°C.  The  synthesis  of  iodopsin  is  com- 
plete within  5  min.,  while  that  of  rhodopsin  continues 
for  well  over  an  hour.  The  data  are  taken  from  the 
same  experiment  as  figure  10  but  with  rhodopsin 
extinctions  multiplied  by  1.3.  It  is  hardly  necessary 
to  labor  the  clo.se  relationship  between  these  measure- 
ments, the  synthesis  of  human  rod  and  cone  pig- 
ments in  vivo,  and  the  course  of  human  dark  adapta- 
tion, cone  and  rod.  Again,  however,  what  parallelism 
obtains  involves  the  comparison  of  log  sensitivity 
with    the    concentration    of   the    visual    pigments. 

One  must  conclude  from  all  these  measurements 
that  light  and  dark  adaptation  have  their  primary 
source  in  the  bleaching  and  resynthesis  of  the  visual 
pigments  of  the  rods  and  cones.  To  be  sure,  more 
central   phenomena — changes   in   the   sensitivities   of 


688 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY   ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOG\-    I 


—I 1 1 1 1 1 \ 1 

Synthesis  of  photo pigmerrts 

from  chicMen  opsins  +  retinene 


O.S  - 


0.4 


03 


0  2 


0.1 


a  - 


I       I       r 


rhodopsin  (SOOm/i ) 


iodopsin  fS60mp) 


Time  in  diarK~nninutas 


A L 


J \ L 


;o 


20 


30 


FIG.  2  1.  Synthesis  of  iodopsin  and  rhodopsin  in  solution  from  a  mixture  of  chicken  opsins  and 
neo-i  retinene.  23°C.  This  is  the  same  experiment  as  shown  in  figure  lo  but  with  the  rhodopsin 
extinctions  multiplied  by  1.3.  At  this  temperature,  iodopsin  synthesis  is  complete  within  2  to  3 
min.,   whereas   rhodopsin   synthesis   still   continues   after   35   min.    [From   W'ald   C72).] 


neurons  and  synapses  along  the  optic  pathways — 
may  also  play  a  role.  Of  this  possibility  as  yet  very 
little  is  known.  In  general,  nein-al  adaptations  are 
relatively  rapid;  if  they  enter  at  all,  they  should 
probably  be  completed  during  the  earliest  stages 
ot  visual  adaptation.  They  probabh'  are  responsible 
al,so  for  only  a  minor  portion  of  the  range  of  \isual 
adaptation.  As  a  first  approximation,  light  and  dark 
adaptation  seem  to  reflect  the  fall  and  rise  of  visual 
pigment;  and  specifically  it  is  the  log  sensitivity  which 
runs  parallel  with  pigment  concentration. 

I  iliiiiiiii  A  Deficiency  and  .A'lghl  Blindness 

Probably  the  earliest  symptom  of  \-itamin  A  de- 
ficiency in  man  and  other  animals  is  the  rise  of  visual 
threshold  known  as  night  blindness.  Because  night 
vision  is  associated  with  the  rods,  it  was  once  thought 
that  dietary  night  blindness,  so  called  to  distinguish 
it  from  the  idiopathic  or  congenital  disease,  is  a 
failure  specifically  of  rod  \ision.  The  first  experi- 
mental studies  of  human  night  blindness,  however, 
showed  at  once  that  in  \itamin  A  deficiency  cone 
vision  deteriorates  with  rod  \ision,  and  both  recover 
together  on  administration  of  \itamin  A  (figs.  22,  23) 
(26,  27,  77). 


The  realization  that  both  iodopsin  and  rhodopsin 
are  synthesized  from  the  same  form  of  vitamin  .\ 
oflfers  a  substantial  theoretical  basis  for  this  relation- 
ship. To  he  sure,  iodopsin  has  not  been  demonstrated 
in  human  cones;  if  present,  it  is  presumably  ac- 
companied by  at  least  two  other  cone  pigments 
needed  to  account  for  normal  human  color  vision. 
\'et  the  observation  that  on  administration  of  \itamin 
.■\,  or  carotene,  night  blindness  is  repaired  as  quickly 
and  completely  in  the  cones  as  in  the  rods  (fig.  23) 
implies  that  the  human  cone  pigments  as  a  group 
are  probabh'  synthesized  from  vitamin  A.  Just  as 
rhodopsin  and  iodopsin  are  constructed  by  joining 
the  same  prosthetic  group  to  different  opsins,  so  the 
cone  pigments  responsible  for  human  color  vision 
may  well  be  composed  of  the  same  retinene  com- 
bined with  a  variety  of  different  opsins. 

The  opsins  ha\e  been  altogether  a  neglected  coiti- 
poncnt  in  the  etiology  of  dietary  night  blindness. 
This  disease  and  its  cure  have  been  thought  of  too 
much  in  terms  of  the  removal  and  replacement  of 
\itamin  A,  particularly  since  vitamin  A  was  shown 
to  be  a  precursor  of  rhodopsin.  This  preconception 
may  be  the  source  of  some  of  the  embarrassments 
that  have  attended  the  experimental  study  of  night 
blindness:   a)    on    beginning    a    vitamin    A-deficienl 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


689 


diet,  some  subjects  immediately  begin  to  become 
night-blind,  whereas  others  show  no  effects,  visual 
or  otherwise,  for  many  months;  and  //)  on  admin- 
istration of  vitamin  A  to  night-blind  subjects,  some 


^ 


1 

'    ' 

1 

7rot«nt       \ 

Con 

» 

1     , 

s_^^ 

>— ^ 

/- 

""i^ 

/] 

\ 

— 

* 

>  ' 

u 

< 

> 

I 

fiods 

Ox' 

^ 

«o 

0, 

^ 

-- 

,  • 

» 

I'^iJfS,"; 

•\ 

i        1 

10  10  30-0  10  iO  JO 

pre-diet  -DAYS -on  diet 


FIG.  22.  Thresholds  of  completely  dark -adapted  cones 
and  rods  during  30  days  of  heavy  vitamin  .\  administration 
(Jejt)  and  during  30  days  on  a  vitamin  .'\-deficient  diet  (jight"). 
Open  and  closed  circles  show  thresholds  of  right  and  left  eyes, 
respectively.  On  the  thirtieth  day  of  the  deficient  diet,  one 
dose  of  vitamin  A  was  administered;  both  rod  and  cone  thresh- 
olds returned  to  normal.  On  the  thirty-second  day,  the  subject 
was  again  slightly  night-blind  and  was  given  a  dose  of  carotene, 
again  both  cone  and  rod  thresholds  returned  to  normal. 
[From  VVald  et  al.  C77).] 


are  cured  completely  within  several  hours,  whereas 
others  retain  some  degree  of  night  blindness  for 
months  while  recei\ing  a  high  dosage  of  vitamin  A. 
Figure  22  shows  the  rapid  type  of  onset  of  night  blind- 
ness, figure  23  the  rapid  type  of  cure.  Unfortunately 
the  other  type  of  result  is  observed  at  least  as  often 
(28,  35,  66). 

One  must  distinguish  an  acute  from  a  chronic 
syndrome  in  \itamin  A  deficiency.  The  results  of  a 
current  study  ot  vitamin  A  deficiency  in  the  rat  are 
summarized  in  figure  24  (cf.  12a).  When  an  animal 
is  placed  on  a  vitamin  A-deficient  diet,  the  liver  stores 
slowly  lose  \itamin  A  until  the  liver  has  been  emptied. 
Up  to  this  time  the  blood  level  remains  normal,  but 
now  it  sinks  within  a  few  days  to  zero.  To  this  point 
the  rhodopsin  content  of  the  retina  has  remained  nor- 
mal, but  now  this  too  falls,  marking  the  beginning  of 
night  blindness.  For  about  three  weeks  longer  the  opsin 
level  stays  normal.  Then  it  too  begins  to  fall;  at  the 
same  time  the  retina  deteriorates  anatomically,  and 
the  animal  loses  weight  and  displays  other  overt  signs 
of  vitamin  A  deficiency.  All  these  disorders  are  reversed 
by  administration  of  \itamin  A. 

The  role  of  vitamin  A  as  the  precursor  of  visual 
pigments  seems  almost  trivial  coinpared  with  its 
general  role  in  maintaining  the  integritv  of  the 
tissues.  The  mechanism  of  this  action  is  still  com- 
pletely obscure.  In  vitamin  A  deficiencv,  various 
tissues   all  over  the   bod\    begin   to  deteriorate,   the 


ao  )oo 

mi n  i/fes 


FIG.  23.  The  cure  of  night-blindness  with  carotene.  Following  a  standard  light  adaptation,  the 
measurement  of  dark  adaptation  shows  both  cone  and  rod  plateaus  to  lie  abose  their  normal  range 
(enclosed  within  the  upper  and  lower  pairs  of  broken  lines^.  After  dark  adaptation  was  completed,  20,000 
International  Units  of  carotene  in  oil  were  administered  in  gelatin  capsules  orally.  For  12  to  14 
min.  the  rod  threshold  remained  constant;  then  it  fell  rapidly  to  normal.  Immediate  repetition  of 
the  standard  adaptation  procedure  showed  both  cone  and  rod  plateaus  to  have  entered  their  normal 
ranges.  [F'rom  Wald  &  .Steven  (78).] 


690 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


too  - 


80- 


V    60 
o 

0) 
0 


R, 


Vitamin  A 

deficient  rats 


live 


5         6         7         8 
WeeKs   on  diet 

FIG.  24.  Vitamin  A  deficiency  in  the  rat.  Blood  vitamin  .\ 
(CD)  falls  precipitously  as  liver  stores  of  vitamin  .-^  (S)  are 
exhausted.  At  this  point,  the  rhodopsin  content  of  the  dark- 
adapted  eye  (O)  begins  to  decline,  presumably  because  not 
enough  vitamin  .\  is  available  to  convert  all  the  opsin  of  the 
rods  to  rhodopsin.  About  three  weeks  later,  opsin  itself  (•) 
begins  to  disappear,  its  concentration  from  then  on  paralleling 
the  decreasing  rhodopsin  content.  Disappearance  of  opsin 
may  in  part  be  responsible  for  the  degeneration  of  rods  and 
cones  associated  with  chronic  vitamin  .-\  deficiency.  [From 
Dowling  &  VVald  (12a).] 


retina  among  others  (36,  54).  Johnson  has  reported 
that  after  7  to  13  weeks  of  vitamin  A  deprivation  in 
young  rats,  the  rods  in  the  retinal  fundus  exhibit 
marked  changes.  Many  outer  segments  have  dis- 
appeared and  those  that  remain  stain  aljnormally. 
As  the  deficiency  progresses,  the  inner  segments  of 
the  rods  also  degenerate,  then  successively  the  ex- 
ternal limiting  membrane  and  outer  nuclear  layer, 
the  pigment  epithelium,  the  outer  molecular  layer, 
and  the  inner  nuclear  layer.  These  changes  progress 
much  faster  in  the  central  retina  than  toward  the 
periphery.  Outer  segments  of  rods  which  have  suffered 
only  slight  degenerative  changes  seem  to  repair 
considerably  within  24  hours  of  vitainin  A  therapy. 
Even  rods  which  have  degenerated  completely 
appear  to  regenerate  within  10  to  18  weeks  of  \itamin 
A  administration. 


The  rod  outer  seginent  is  composed  in  considerable 
measure  of  rhodopsin  (see  above).  A  loss  of  opsin 
might  therefore  profoundly  damage  its  structural 
integrity;  long  before  such  changes  are  visible  in 
the  micro.scope  they  might  become  detectable  physi- 
ologically as  night  blindness.  In  any  case,  night 
blindness  clearly  involves  far  more  than  the  simple 
decline  of  \itamin  A  concentration  in  the  retina. 
It  introduces,  particularly  in  prolonged  deficiency, 
deep-seated  anatomical  changes  and  these  might 
repair  only  very  slowly. 

In  addition  to  deficiency  in  the  diet,  any  inter- 
ference with  the  flow  of  vitamin  A  to  the  retina,  or 
with  its  uiilization  by  the  tissues  can  be  expected  to 
react  on  the  visual  threshold.  This  appears  to  be  the 
case  in  certain  chronic  liver  diseases  (cf.  41).  Bile 
is  needed  for  the  absorption  of  both  carotene  and 
\itamin  A  (25).  In  obstructi\e  jaundice,  in  which 
bile  fails  to  reach  the  intestine,  vitamin  A  deficiency 
and  hence  night  Ijlindness  may  develop  in  spite  of  a 
diet  adecjuate  to  meet  normal  requirements.  In  ad- 
dition to  producing  the  bile,  the  li\er  is  the  principal 
storage  tissue  for  vitamin  .\.  It  is  not  surprising 
therefore  that  liver  disorders  may  affect  the  extent, 
and  apparently  in  some  instances  also  the  rate,  of 
dark  adaptation. 

Recently  it  has  been  shown  that  the  wall  of  the 
intestine  is  probably  the  principal  site  for  the  con- 
version of  carotene  to  \itamin  A  (14,  55).  It  is  not 
unlikelv  that  conditions  exist  in  which  some  failure 
of    this    process    leads    to    \isual    disturbances. 

Even  when  the  diet  is  adequate,  and  the  liver  and 
intestine  are  performing  their  functions,  this  may 
not  yet  be  enough.  \'ision  depends,  not  merely  on 
\itamin  A,  but  on  a  particular  shape  of  vitamin  A, 
the  nco-h  isomer.  This  is  not  ordinarily  present  in 
the  food,  so  that  other  isomers  of  vitamin  A  obtained 
in  the  diet  must  be  converted  into  this  special  con- 
figuration. The  neo-fe  isomer  is  continuously  lost 
in  the  bleaching  of  the  visual  pigments  and  must  be 
continuously  replaced  for  vision  to  persist.  It  is  not 
impossible  that  there  exists  a  visual  disorder  that 
has  its  source  in  a  failure  to  isomerize  vitamin  A. 
Furthermore,  the  fact  that  \itamin  A  is  stored  in  the 
eye  as  an  ester,  which  must  presumably  be  hy- 
drolyzed  before  entering  the  \isual  cycle,  constitutes 
another  point  at  which  \isual  processes  are  vulnerable 
to  metabolic  failure. 

It  has  repeatedly  been  suggested  that  retinitis 
pigmentosa,  a  degenerative  disease  which  attacks 
primarily  the  layer  of  rods  and  cones,  is  due  to  some 


THE    PHOTORECEPTOR    PROCESS    IN    VISION 


691 


local  failure  in  the  supply  or  effective  utilization  of 
vitamin  A  (9,  81,  82).  The  lesions  at  one  stage  of 
vitamin  A  deficiency  resemble  somewhat  those  in 
retinitis  pigmentosa.  The  layer  of  rods  and  cones  is 
the  first  to  deteriorate  in  vitamin  A  deficiency,  and 
such  deterioration  is  characteristic  of  the  disease. 
In  more  advanced  vitamin  A  deficiency,  however, 
the  inner  retinal  layers  also  succumb,  while  remaining 
apparently  intact  in  retinitis  pigmentosa.  It  is  con- 
ceivable that  these  symptoms  are  due  to  a  local 
failure  in  vitamin  A  metabolism  which  is  not  ap- 
parent elsewhere  in  the  eye  tissues. 

The  participation  of  vitamin  A  in  the  processes 
of  visual  excitation  therefore  introduces  a  whole 
series  of  special  relationships.  It  renders  vision  de- 
pendent upon  an  ecological  factor,  the  nutrition, 
and  upon  the  entire  network  of  internal  arrangements 


that  govern  the  absorption,   metabolism  and   trans- 
port of  vitamin  A  throughout  the  body. 

.\  icotinamide 

A  second  \itamhi  plays  a  basic  role  in  the  \isual 
processes:  nicotinamide,  the  anti-pellagra  factor  of 
the  \itamin  B  complex  and  the  active  principle  of 
DPN,  which  is  the  coenzyme  of  the  alcohol  dehy- 
drogenase system.  Without  this  factor  vitamin  A 
presumably  cannot  be  oxidized  to  retinene,  a  neces- 
sary step  in  the  synthesis  of  rhodopsin  and  iodopsin. 

Are  there  visual  symptoms  in  pellagra?  Is  there, 
for  example,  some  disturbance  of  dark  adaptation 
in  this  disease?  None  has  been  reported;  but  it  might 
be  well  to  examine  carefully  the  visual  beha\ior  of 
pellagrins  with  these  considerations  in  mind. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Albrecht,  G.  S.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  In  press. 

2.  AUERBACH,  E.  AND  G.  Wald.  Am.  J.  Ophlli.  39:  24,  1955. 

3.  Ball,  S.,  T.  W.  Goodwin  and  R.  .\.  Morton.  Biochem. 
J.  42:  516,  1948. 

4.  Bliss,   A.   F.   Arch.   Biochem.    &   Biophys.    31:    197,    1951. 

5.  Boll,  F.  Arch.  Anal.  &  Physiol.:  4,  1877. 

6.  BouMAN,  M.  A.  and  H.  A.  van  der  Velden.  J.  Opt.  Soc. 
Am.  37:908,  1947. 

7.  Broda,  E.  E.  and  E.  Victor.  Biochem.  J.  34:  1501,   1940. 

8.  Campbell,    F.   W.    and   W.   A.    H.    Rushton.   J.    Physiol. 

■30:  '3'.  '955- 

9.  CocAN,  D.  G.   Tr.  Am.  Acad.  Ophlh.:  629,  July-.'\ug.  1950. 
ga. Denton,  E.  J.  .\'at.  Phys.  Lab.  A.  K.  Proc.  Symp.  8.  In  press. 

10.  Dieterle,  J.   M.   AND   C.   D.   Robeson.   Science    120:   219, 

■954- 

11.  DoNNER,  K.  O.  J.  Physiol.  122:  524,  1953. 

12.  DoNNER,   K.    O.    AND   R.    Gr.\nit.   Ada  physiol.   scandinav. 
17:  161,  1949. 

laa.DowLiNG,  J.  E.  and  G.  W'ald.  Proc.  .\al.  Acad.  Sc,  H'ash- 
ington  44:  648,  1958. 

13.  Farrar,  K.  R.,  J.  C.  Hamlet,  H.  B.  Henbest  and  E.  R. 
H.  Jones.  Chem.  &  Indust.:  49,  1951. 

14.  Glover,  J.,  T.  W.  Goodwin  and  R.  A.  Morton.  Biochem. 
J.  Proc,  41  :  xiv,  1947. 

15.  Granit,  R.  .Ada  physiol.  scandinav.   i  :  386,  1941. 

16.  Granit,  R.  .Ada  physiol .  scandinav.  ■2:  334,  1941- 

17.  Granit,  R.  .icia  physiol.  scandinav.  3:  137,  1942. 

18.  Granit,  R.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  3:  318,  1942. 

19.  Granit,  R.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  4:  118,  1942. 

20.  Granit,  R.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  5:  108,  1943. 

21.  Granit,  R.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  5;  219,  1943. 

22.  Granit,    R.    Sensory    .Mechanisms    of    the    Retina.    London: 
0.\ford,  1947,  p.  292. 

23.  Granit,  R.,  T.  Holmberg  and  M.  Zewi.  J.  Physiol.  94: 
430,  1938. 

24.  Granit,  R.,  A.  Munsterhjelm  and  M.  Zewi.  J.  Physiol. 

96:  31.  '939- 

25.  Greaves,  J.  D.  and  C.   I.   A.   Schmidt.   Am.  J.   Physiol. 
111:492,  1935. 


26.  Haig,  C,  S.  Hecht  and  \.  J.  Patek,  Jr.  Science  87;  534, 
>938- 

27.  Hecht,   S.    and  J.    Mandelbaum.   Science  88:   219,    1938. 

28.  Hecht,  S.  and  J.  Mandelbaum.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  130; 
651,  1940, 

29.  Hecht,  S.,  S.  Shlaer  and  M.  H.  Pirenne.  J.  Gen.  Physiol. 
25:  819,  i94>-42- 

30.  Hubbard,  R.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  37:  381,  1953-54. 

31.  Hubbard,  R.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  935,  1955-56. 
31a. Hubbard,  R.  Nature,  London  181:  1126,  1958. 

32.  Hubbard,  R.,  R.  I.  Gregerman  and  G.  \V.\ld.  J.  Gen. 
Physiol.  36:  415,  1952-53. 

33.  Hubb.ard,  R.  .\nd  G.  Wald.  Proc.  .\at.  Acad.  Sc,  Wash- 
ington 37:69,  1 95 1. 

34.  Hubbard,    R.    and    G.    Wald.   J.    Gen.    Physiol.    36:    269, 

■95^-53- 

35.  Hume,  E.  M.  and  H.  .•\.  Krebs.  Vitamin  A  Requirements  of 
Human  Adults  (Medical  Res.  Council,  Special  Report 
Series  No.   264).  London:  His  Majesty's  Stat.  Off.,   1949. 

36.  Johnson,  M.  L.  A.M. A.  Arch.  Ophth.  29:  793,  1943. 

37.  Krinskv,  N.  I.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  In  press. 

38.  KiJHNE,  \V.  In:  Handbuch  der  Physiologic,  edited  by  L. 
Hermann.    Leipzig:    Vogel,    1879,    vol.    3,    pt.    i,    p.    312. 

39.  Oroshnik,  W.  J.  -tm.  Chem.  Soc.  78:  2651,  1956. 

40.  Oroshnik,  \V.,  P.  K.  Brown,  R.  Hubbard  .\nd  G.  Wald. 
Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sc,  Washington  42:  578,  1956. 

41.  Patek,  A.  J.  and  C.  Haig.  J.  Clin.  Invest.   18:  609,   1939. 

42.  Pauling,   L.   Fortschr.   Chem.  org.  Naturstoffe  3:  203,    1939. 

43.  Pauling,    L.    Helvet.    ckim.    acta    32:    2241,    1949. 

44.  Pirenne,  M.  H.  Brit.  .M.  Bull.  9:  61,  1953. 

45.  Radding,  C.   M.   and  G.  Wald.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  909, 

'955-56- 

46.  R.\DDING,  C.  M.   and  G.  Wald.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  39:  923, 

1955-56- 

47.  Robeson,   C.    D.    and  J.    G.    Baxter.   J.   Am.   Chem.   Soc. 

69:  136,  1947- 

48.  Robeson,   C.   D.,   W.   P.   Blum,  J.   M.   Dieterle,  J.   D. 


692 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HVSIOLOGV 


NEURDPHYSIOLOCl'    I 


Cawley  and  J.  G.  Baxter.  J.   Am.  Cfiem.  Soc.  77:  4120, 

1955- 

49.  RusHTON,  \V.  A.  H.  Nature,  London  179:  571,  1957. 

50.  RusHTON,  \V.   A.   H.,  F.  W.   Campbell,  \V.  A.   Higgins 
AND  G.  S.  Brindle^'.  Optica  acta  i  :  1B3,  1955. 

51.  RusHTON,   W.   A.    H.   AND   R.    D.    Cohen.   Nature,   London 

173:  301.  ■954- 

52.  SjosTRAND,   F.   S.  J.   Celt.   &   Comp.   Pfiysiol.   42:    15,    1953. 

53.  Stiles,    \V.    S.    .Nederl.    tijdschr.    Natuurk.     15:    125,    1949. 

54.  Tanslev,  K.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  ser.  B  114:  79,  1933. 

55.  Thompson,  S.  Y.,  J.  Gangulv  and  S.   K.  Kon.  Bnt.  J. 
Nutrition  3:50,  1 949. 

56.  VVald,  G.  J.  Gen.  Physwl.  19:  781,  1935-36. 

57.  VVald,  G.  Nature,  London  139:  1017,  1937. 

58.  VVald,  G.  Nature,  London  140:  545,  1937. 

59.  Wald,  G.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  21  :  795,  1937-38. 

60.  VVald,  G.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  775,  1938-39. 

61.  VVald,  G.  Science  loi  :  653,  1945. 

62.  VVald,  G.  Harvey  Lectures.  Ser.  41  :  117,  1945-46. 

63.  Wald,  G.  Docum.  ophth.  3:  94,  1949. 

64.  Wald,  G.  Biochimi.  et  biophys.  acta  4:  215,  1950. 

65.  Wald,  G.  Science  iig:  887,  1954. 

66.  Wald,  G.,  L.  Brouha  and  R.  E.  Johnson.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

i37:.'J5'.  1942- 

67.  Wald,  G.  and  P.  K.   Brown.  Proc.  .\al.  Acad.  Sc,   Wash- 
ington 36:  84,  1950. 

68.  Wald,   G.    and    P.    K.    Brown.   J.    Gen.    Physiol.    35:   797, 

■95I-52- 


69.  Wald,   G.    and    P.    K.    Brown.  J.   Gen.    Physiol.    37:    189, 

'953-54- 

70.  Wald,  G.  and  P.  K..  Brown.  Nature,  London  177;  174,  1956. 
70a. Wald,  G.  and  P.  K.  Brown.  Science  127:  222,  1958. 

71.  Wald,   G.,   P.   K.   Brown   and   P.   H.   .Smith.   Science    118: 

505.  1953- 

72.  Wald,  G.,  P.  K.  Brown  and  P.  H.  .Smith.  J.  Gen.  Physiol. 

38:  623,  1954-55. 

73.  Wald,  G.  and  A.  B.  Clark.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  21  :  93,  1937-38. 

74.  Wald,  G.,  J.  Durell  and  R.  C.  C.  .St.  George.  Science 

11'  :  179.  1950- 

75.  Wald,    G.    and    R.    Hubb.ard.   J.    Gen.    Physiol.    32 :    367, 

1948-49- 

76.  Wald,   G.  and  R.   Hubbard.   Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sc,    Wash- 
ington 36:  92,  1950. 

77.  Wald,   G.,   H.  Jeghers  and  J.   .Arminio.   Am.  J.  Physiol. 

1^3:  732,  '938- 

78.  Wald,  G.  and  D.  Steven.  Proc.  Nat.  Acad.  Sc,  Washington 

25:  344.  1939- 

79.  Wald,  G.  and  H.  Zussman.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  122:  449,  1938. 

80.  Walls,  G.  L.  and  H.  D.  Judd.  Brit.  J.  Ophlh.   17:  641, 

705>  '933- 

81.  Yudkin,  a.  M.  J.  A.  M.  A.  191  :  921,  1933. 

82.  Ze.avin,  B.  H.  and  G.  Wald.  Am.  J.  Ophth.  42:  part  II; 

254.  1956. 

83.  Zechmeister,  L.  Chem.  Rev.  34;  267,  1944. 

84.  Zechmeister,  L.  Experientia  10:  i,  19.54. 


CHAPTER    XXIX 


Neural  activity  in  the  retina 


RAGNAR     GRANIT      1      Nnhel  hulitute  for  Neurophysiology,  KaroUnska  Instituli-t,  Stockholm,  Sweden 


CHAPTER     C:  O  N  T  E  N  T  S 

Outline  of  Retinal  Histology 

Electroretinogram  (ERG) 

Neural  Patterns 

Stimulus  Correlates 

Centrifugal  Control 

ERG  of  Man:  Its  Clinical  Use 


OUTLINE  OF  RETINAL  HISTOLOGY 

THE  RETINA  consists  of  a  surface  layer  of  receptors, 
the  rods  and  cones  (fig.  i),  joined  to  a  nervous  center 
which  delivers  an  organized  message  in  terms  of  im- 
pulses through  the  optic  nerve.  The  great  works  of 
Ramon  y  Cajal  (123,  124)  and  Polyak  (122)  should 
be  consulted  for  details.  The  anatomy  of  the  eye  and 
retina  throughout  the  vertebrates  has  been  ably 
discussed  by  Walls  (146).  There  is  also  a  recent  brief 
summary  by  Willmer  (147). 

In  lower  vertebrates  it  is  not  always  easy  to  dis- 
tinguish rods  from  cones  (38,  146).  In  mammals  rods 
end  in  knobs  and  cones  in  dendrites,  but  in  frogs  both 
types  of  receptor  have  dendritic  terminals.  Rods  are 
generally  more  elongated  and  slender  than  cones  but 
this  criterion  breaks  down  in  some  lizards  and  birds 
and  in  the  fovea  of  the  primates  in  which  the  elongated 
cones  look  like  rods.  Walls  emphasizes  that  the  outer 
cone  segment  is  enclosed  by  a  tubular  process  from 
the  pigment  epithelium  cell  oppo.site  to  it  and  holds 
this  criterion  to  be  universal  and  never  found  in  rods. 
Differentiation  between  rods  and  cones  seems  pos- 
sible by  electron  microscopy,  at  least  in  some  species 
(132,  133,  134,  135).  It  has  even  been  possible  to 
distinguish  two  kinds  of  rods  in  guinea  pigs  (135) 
which  would  agree  well  with  the  electrophysiological 


observations  on  blue  sensiti\ity  existing  in  this  spe- 
cies which  has  almost  no  cones.  Photodichroism,  an 
orientation  of  the  light-absorbing  molecules  serving 
to  aid  absorption,  has  been  observed  in  the  rods  (36, 
1 29).  The  fresh  cones,  viewed  end  on,  light  up  when 
the  micro.scope  is  focused  on  the  outer  limbs  which 
thus  seem  to  serve  as  a  focusing  device  operating  by 
total  internal  reflection  (138).  This  observation  may 
explain  why  a  pencil  of  light  entering  at  an  angle  is 
dimmed  if  it  enters  cones,  the  Stiles-Crawford  effect 

(136). 

Double  cones  and  twin  cones  have  been  described 
in  fish  but  since  these  types  do  not  occur  in  mammals, 
they  have  attracted  little  attention,  physiological  work 
rather  tending  to  settle  on  universal  characteristics. 
Schwalbes  green'  rods  found  in  frogs  have  recently 
been  observed  to  contain  a  special  blue-absorbing 
photosensitive  substance  (37)  and  ma\'  well  be  more 
general  than  one  had  thought. 

The  rods  are  integrating  organs  and  converge  in 
large  numbers  towards  the  bipolar  dendrites.  Bipolars 
in  their  turn  converge  towards  the  ganglion  cells 
which  give  rise  to  the  optic  nerve  fibers.  In  man  there 
are  some  125,000,000  rods  as  against  800,000  to  1,000,- 
000  optic  nerve  fibers.  Since  man  has  only  4  to  7  mil- 
lion cones,  it  is  clear  that  the  amount  of  convergence 
is  far  less  for  them;  this  criterion  seems  to  be  general 
throughout  the  animal  kingdom,  signifying  that 
cones,  on  the  whole,  are  more  discriminative,  while 
rods  are  more  integrative  and  designed  to  serve  as 
collectors  of  light  quanta  in  the  dark  (92,  127).  The 
fundamental  observation  that  rods  actually  do  dom- 
inate the  eyes  of  nocturnal  animals  and  that  an  in- 
creasing number  of  cones  is  characteristic  of  diurnal 
habits  was  made  by  Schultze  (131)  on  the  basis  of 
extensive  histological  studies.  This,  and  later  psycho- 
physical work  by  Parinaud  (117),  Konig  (99)  and 


693 


694 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOCn-    I 


i 


B 


m 


^n     -I 


/        it  n 

f        c 


e 

4/ 


t 


FIG.  I.  A.  Types  of  cones  from  the  leopard  frog,  Rana  pipieris  (c);  the  snapping  turtle,  Chelydra 
serpentina  (</);  the  marsh  hawk,  Circus  hudsoniits  (c);  and  from  the  circumfoveal  eminence  of  man 
(/).  B.  Types  of  rods  from  the  goldfish,  Carassius  auratus  (/);  the  leopard  frog,  a  common  or  'red' 
rod  ((/);  the  leopard  frog,  a  so-called  green'  rod  of  Schwalbe  (f);  the  flying  squirrel,  Glaucomys 
V.  volans  (/);  and  from  the  temporal  side  of  the  macula  lutea  of  man  C?)-  [From  Walls  (146).] 


von  Kries  (145)  led  to  the  concept  of  a  duplex  retina 
for  scotopic  and  photopic  vision,  respectively.  Most 
eyes  from  this  point  of  view  are  'mixed',  that  is  they 
contain  two  organs  in  one.  When,  at  about  the  same 
period,  Boll's  (24)  discovery  of  the  light-sensitive 
visual  purple  (rhodopsin)  was  made  and  ably  elab- 
orated by  Kiihne  (loi),  Konig  (99)  and  their  col- 
laborators, this  gave  further  support  to  the  duplicity 
theory.  Visual  purple  has  been  found  only  in  the 
outer  limbs  of  the  rod.  A  historical  review  of  this 
development  is  available  (69). 

In  the  external  plexiform  layer,  between  the  recep- 
tors and  bipolar  cells,  there  are  lateral  connections, 
the  horizontal  cells  (fig.  2),  joining  cones,  each  of 
which  is  embraced  by  a  dendritic  basket,  to  a  larger 
group  of  rods  and  cones.  There  are  several  baskets 
to  each  horizontal  cell  and  axons  up  to  0.8  mm  in 
length  have  been  found  (123).  The  arrangement  sug- 


gests a  starting  loop  or  positive  feedback  for  general 
facilitation. 

Polyak's  (122)  classification  of  bipolars  is  of  interest 
because  it  is  based  on  primates.  There  is,  in  the  foveal 
area,  the  midget  bipolar  which  is  individual  or  private 
for  a  single  cone.  In  the  periphery  each  midget  bi- 
polar receives  impulses  from  a  small  number  of  cones. 
At  the  opposite  end  it  articulates  with  a  midget 
ganglion  cell  by  an  axodendritic  synapse,  yet  this 
midget  system  is  not  wholly  isolated.  Mop  bipolars 
also  run  to  the  midget  ganglion  but  this  contact  is 
axosomatic.  These  together  with  all  the  other  bipolar 
types  belong  to  the  diffuse  variety  which  receive  a 
large  number  of  receptors.  The  mop  bipolars  possess 
a  kind  of  dendritic  tuft,  smaller  in  the  fovea  than  in 
the  periphery  and  forming  a  receptaculum  for  rod 
and  cone  pedicles.  Its  axosomatic  projection  is  a  crude 
shallow  basket  touching  one  or  more  ganglion  cell 


NEURAL    ACTIVIT"!'    IN    THE    RETINA 


695 


FIG.  2.  A.  Scheme  of  the  structures  of  the  primate  retina  as  revealed  by  the  method  of  Golgi. 
The  layers  and  the  zones  are  designated  as  follows:  (/)  pigment  layer;  (,2-a)  outer  zone  and  (^-A) 
inner  zone  of  the  rod  and  cone  layer;  (5)  outer  limiting  membrane;  (_4-a)  outer  zone  and  (^-A) 
inner  zone  of  the  outer  nuclear  layer;  (is-a)  outer  zone,  (j-A)  middle  zone  and  (j-c)  inner  zone 
of  the  outer  plexiform  layer;  (5)  inner  nuclear  layer  with  its  four  zones;  (7)  inner  plexiform  layer; 
C5)  layer  of  the  ganglion  cells;  (9)  layer  of  the  optic  nerve  fibers;  and  (/o)  inner  limiting  membrane. 
The  nerve  cells  are  designated  as  follows:  (a)  rods,  (A)  cones,  (r)  horizontal  cells,  (d,  e,  f,  A)  bi- 
polar cells,  (i,  /)  so-called  'amacrine  cells',  (m,  n,  0,  p,  i)  ganglion  cells  and  00  'radial  fibers'  of 
Miiller.  In  this  scheme  the  nervous  elements  are  reduced  to  their  essentials,  with,  however,  the 
characteristic  features  of  each  variety  preserved — the  location  of  the  cell  bodies,  the  size,  the  shape, 
and  the  spreading  of  the  dendrites  and  of  the  axis  cylinders — and  with  the  synaptic  contacts  pre- 
sented accurately.  [From  Polyak  (122}.] 

B.  The  structure  of  the  primate  retina  reduced  to  its  essentials,  including  the  synopsis  of  the 
propagation  of  the  retinal  impulses  from  the  photoreceptors  to  other  parts  of  the  retina,  to  the 
brain,  and  from  the  brain  back  to  the  retina  (direction  indicated  by  the  arrows).  The  marking 
of  the  layers  and  the  zones  the  same  as  in  A.  Labeling  of  the  cells:  (a.  A)  rods  and  cones,  the  pho- 
toreceptors where  the  nervous  impulses  are  generated  by  physical  'light'  (in  the  scheme  only  the 
left  group  of  the  photoreceptors  is  assumed  to  be  stimulated  by  light);  (c)  horizontal  cells  which 
transmit  the  impulses  to  the  surrounding  rods  and  cones;  (_d,  c,  /,  A)  centripetal  bipolar  cells  of 
the  mop,  brush,  flat  and  midget  varieties,  which  'transmit'  the  impulses  from  the  photoreceptors 
to  the  ganglion  cells,  the  bipolars  serving  as  'analyzers';  (i)  centrifugal  bipolar  cell,  a  variety  of 
the  'amacrine  cells,'  which  probably  receives  the  impulses  from  the  centripetal  bipolars  from  the 
ganglion  cells,  and  also  from  the  brain  by  way  of  the  centrifugal  or  efferent  fibers  (/)  and  trans- 
mits them  back  upon  the  photoreceptors  (a.  A);  (/)  an  'amacrine  cell'  which  possibly  intercepts 
a  part  of  the  bipolar  impulses  and  spreads  them  over  the  surrounding  territory;  and  (m,  n,  0,  p,  j) 
ganglion  cells  which  receive  impulses  from  the  centripetal  bipolars  and  transmit  them  to  the  brain 
along  their  axon  called   'optic   nerve  fibers.'   [From   Polyak  (122).] 


bodies.  The  brush  and  flat  bipolars  reseinble  each 
other  and  occur  everywhere  in  the  retina  from  the 
fovea  to  the  ora  serrata.  They  have  large  dendritic 
territories.  Their  most  interesting  properties  seem  to 
be:  a)  a  "reciprocal  overlapping  of  each  of  the  den- 
dritic territories  with  its  own  kind"  (122)  and  6) 
axodendritic  articulations  with  the  ganglion  cells. 
The  midget  system  also  intermingles  with  this  wide 


a.xodendritic  or  plexiform  (inner  plexiform  layer) 
network.  The  basic  pattern  consists  of  bipolar  ter- 
minals, ganglion  cell  dendrites  and  the  Golgi  type  II 
of  cells  called  amacrines.  Similar  large  plexiform  net- 
works with  Golgi  type  II  of  cells  are  found  elsewhere 
in  the  nervous  system,  including  the  cortex  of  the 
cerebellum. 

The  ganglion  cells,  for  physiological  correlations. 


696 


HANDB(50K    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


can  be  divided  into  two  types,  large  and  small,  with 
two  extremes,  giant  and  midget.  It  is  doubtful 
whether  the  midget  system  occurs  in  the  common 
laboratory  animals.  The  dendritic  expansions  of  the 
giant  ganglion  cells  may  be  "from  250  to  350  n  across 
and  probably  more"  (Polyak). 

The  system  of  amacrine  cells  (i.e.  cells  without 
axons)  seems  highly  organized.  This,  according  to 
Ramon  y  Cajal,  is  particularK  true  for  the  stratified 
ones  (Polyak's  knotty  amacrines)  which  form  five  to 
seven  layers  in  the  inner  plexiform  network.  Their 
dendritic  arborizations  are  similarly  stratified  at  cor- 
responding levels.  There  are  also  giant  amacrines 
(Polyak's  tasseled  amacrines)  some  of  which  spread 
a  'daddy  longlegs'  mop  extending  over  i  mm  above 
the  plane  of  the  ganglion  cells.  Polyak  has  also  de- 
tected axons  running  from  amacrine  cells  towards  the 
pedicles  of  the  receptors  and  regards  them  as  bipolar 
cells  conducting  backwards.  This  raises  the  question 
of  whether  amacrines  appear  to  lack  axons  merely 
because  of  difficulties  in  staining. 

In  this  microcosm  of  a  nervous  center  that  we  call 
a  retina,  the  inner  plexiform  layer,  as  we  have  seen, 
is  the  meeting  ground  of  three  major  systems  and  thus 
a  critical  region.  It  is  difficult  to  imagine  this  layer 
to  be  wholly  self-controlled  lay  the  chance  play  of 
light  and  shadow.  And,  as  a  matter  of  fact,  this  is  the 
very  region  to  which  the  centrifugal  fibers  of  Ramon 
y  Cajal  (123)  and  Dogiel  (50)  were  found  to  project. 
Ramon  y  Cajal  studied  them  in  the  retina  of  the  dog 
(fig.  3),  while  Dogiel  worked  on  birds.  They  seem 
to  be  difficult  to  stain  and  their  origin  is  unknown, 
yet  Ramon  \  Cajal  did  not  hesitate  to  postulate  a 
central  origin  rather  than  to  describe  them  as  re- 
current collaterals.  Some  centrifugal  fibers  are  held 
to  go  as  far  as  to  the  outer  plexiform  layer. 

The  briefest  path  in  the  retina  clearly  is  disynaptic : 
receptor-bipolar-ganglion.  A  more  fundamental  issue 
seems  to  be  the  question  of  whether  bipolar  cells 
make  axosomatic  or  axodendritic  connections  with 
the  ganglion  cells.  Conduction  is  slow  in  dendrites 
(104)  so  that  axo.somatic  latencies  are  likely  to  be 
shorter.  In  the  probable  absence  of  midget  cells  in 
the  common  laboratory  animals,  the  size  of  the 
ganglion  cell  is  likely  to  be  an  important  property 
because  the  larger  the  cell,  the  greater  the  probability 
of  axodendritic  activation  in  the  inner  plexiform 
layer.  Actually  the  ganglion  spikes  in  the  cat's  retina 
fall  into  two  main  categories,  large  and  small,  the 
small  ones  as  a  rule  having  brief  latent  periods.  The 


larger  the  spike  caused  by  illumination,  the  later  it 
tends  to  be  discharged  and  the  lower  its  absolute 
threshold  to  light.  This  is  the  author's  general  im- 
pression, not  a  result  of  systematic  analysis. 

If,  in  the  cat's  eye,  one  proceeds  to  send  an  anti- 
dromic (backward)  shock  into  the  optic  nerve  and 
places  a  microelectrode  on  the  blind  spot  (74),  the 
volley  recorded  consists  of  an  early  large  and  a  later 
small  group  of  spikes,  similar  to  those  recorded  ortho- 
dromically  at  the  central  end  of  the  optic  nerve  (22, 
23,  107).  The  maximal  conduction  velocities  of  its 
fillers  are  70  and  23  m  per  sec,  respectively  (22).  Two 
main  fiber  sizes  (as  judged  from  the  conduction  veloci- 
ties) suggest  two  main  groups  of  sizes  of  ganglion  cells. 
This  is  further  evidence  for  subdi\'iding  the  spikes  into 
two  main  categories. 

At  the  blind  spot  the  optic  nerve  loses  its  myelin 
sheath  and  so  conduction  suddenly  slows  down  as  the 
antidromic  impulse  enters  the  fibers  running  across 
the  retinal  surface  (74).  Precise  measurements  by 
Dodt  (42)  gave  mean  values  of  2.9  and  1.7  m  per 
sec.  for  large  and  small  spikes,  respectively.  The  large 
spikes  (see  below)  are  the  ones  most  easily  influenced 
by  centrifugal  tetani  (74)  as  also  seems  probable  con- 
sidering their  wide  dendritic  expansions  within  the 
inner  plexiform  layer. 

Another  interesting  point  is  that,  on  account  of  the 
slow  conduction  across  the  retinal  surface,  the  im- 
pulses from  the  peripheral  portions  of  the  retina  may 
i^e  delayed  by  4  to  6  msec,  as  compared  with  those 
arising  in  the  region  around  the  blind  spot.  This  is  of 
technical  interest  because  it  means  that,  unless  special 
precautions  are  taken  in  studying  retinal  brain  pro- 
jections by  evoked  potentials,  these  are  likely  to  be 
mainly  determined  i)y  the  fibers  around  the  blind 
spot.  Phvsiologically  the  delayed  conduction  means 
that,  with  a  moving  retina,  space  coordinates  stand 
a  good  chance  of  being  transformed  into  time  co- 
ordinates. The  eye  always  makes  small  oscillations 
in  fixation  (39,  125). 


ELECTRORETINOGR.KM  (eRG) 

The  electroretinogram  is  a  polyphasic  mass  re- 
sponse (fig.  4)  with  specific  cornea-positive  deflections 
at  the  onset  and  cessation  of  illumination.  Standard 
leads  in  electroretinography  are  between  the  cornea 
and  an  'indifferent'  point  on  the  body  or  behind  the 
bulb  (in  the  case  of  eyes  excised  from  cold-blooded 


NEURAL    ACTIVITY    IN    THE    RETINA 


69/ 


FIG.  3.  .1.  Retina  of  the  dog  showing  cone  axons  (n);  rod  axons  (A);  types  of  bipolars  (c-c).  of 
which  e  is  Ramon  y  Cajals  cone  bipolar;  ganglion  cells  (m,  n);  ascending  nerve  fiber  0);  and  cen- 
trifugal fibers  (j).  B.  Details  of  structure  of  ganglion  cells  (B,  C  and  E")  and  of  connections  made 
by  centrifugal  fibers  (a).  [From  Ramon  y  Cajal  (123).] 


animals).  The  ERG  was  discovered  by  Holmgren  (94, 
95).  The  literature  has  been  twice  summarized  by  the 
author  (69,  73),  the  first  time  with  a  full  historical 
review.  Some  of  the  more  important  classical  papers 
are  those  of  Kiihne  &  Steiner  (102,  103),  Gotch  (60, 
61),  Piper  (119,  120,  121),  Einthoven  &  Jolly  (53), 
Frolich  (56),  Chaffee  et  al.  (31),  Chaffee  &  Hampson 
(32),  Hartline  (85),  Adrian  &  Matthews  (3)  and 
Kohlrausch  (98). 

The  ERG  (which  in  such  animals  as  cats  and  frogs 
reaches  maximal  cornea-positive  values  around  i  mv) 
begins  with  a  small  negative  dip,  the  a-wave,  then 
goes  positive,  the  6-wave.  If  stimulus  intensity  is  suf- 
ficiently high,  there  follows  a  very  slow  cornea-positive 
secondary  rise  or  c-wave  and,  at  the  cessation  of  il- 


lumination, another  positive  hump,  the  off-effect  or 
(/-wave  (see  fig.  4).  There  is  some  doubt  as  to  whether 
the  c-wave  occurs  in  cone  eyes.  In  mixed  eyes  it  is  not 
found  in  the  state  of  light  adaptation  (144).  Noell 
(114)  appears  to  hold  that  it  is  always  present  but 
sometimes  compensated  for  i3\  an  opposite  negative 
potential  of  similar  slow  characteristics.  There  is  no 
reason  to  believe  any  of  the  ERG  waves  to  be  absent 
in  any  kind  of  vertebrate  eye;  but  they  are  very  dif- 
ferently developed  with  respect  to  .size  and  rate  of  rise 
and  they  vary  with  the  experimental  conditions  so 
that,  for  instance,  in  rod  eyes  the  (/-wave  is  small  or 
missing.  The  ERG  has  generally  been  thought  to 
consist  of  components  integrated  in  complex  inter- 
ference   pictures.    These   are   reasonably  well-known 


6g8  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


50  cy/5ec. 

.■^.■■..^■^. 


O.SmV 


...,I,„.I.,,.I....I-.I..,.I....I.„.Im..(,.... 

I 
0  0.5  7.0  sec. 


•Mt||Ul!lM"'<"'<"mM'>>'*MlMlf)(iif|M< 

)l(llllllllil)lllltlllllUIII>ttlHttllltl>i<liii. 


03  nN 


0.5  TO  sec 


FIG.  4.  Elcctroretinograms  from  various  types  of  retinas.  A.  Cone  retina  of  the  cold  blooded 
horned  toad,  Phyronosoma,  showing  a  large  diphasic  (/-wave.  [From  Chaffee  &  Sutcliffe  (33).]  B. 
Mammalian  cone  retina  of  the  squirrel,  Sciurus  carolinensis  leucotis,  with  marked  a-wave  and  narrow 
pointed  b-  and  (/-waves  (calibration:  0.5  mv;  time,  i  sec).  [From  Arden  &  Tansley  (8)]  C.  Cat 
eye,  dark-adapted,  exposed  to  two  flash  durations  at  an  intensity  of  about  700  meter-candles; 
(!-wave  is  just  visible,  A-wave  shows  fast  oscillation,  followed  by  a  drop  below  base  line  before  the 
c-vjavc  begins;  (/-wave  or  off -effect  appears  as  a  retarding  of  fall  of  response  at  cessation  of  illumi- 
nation. [From  Granit  (73).]  D.  Guinea  pig  eye,  dark-adapted,  exposed  to  light  intensity  of  about 
goo  lux,  showing  a  definite  (j-wave  and  indication  of  double  ft-wave;  in  this  eye  the  c-wave  or  sec- 
ondary rise  tends  to  be  the  most  prominent  phase  of  the  response.  [From  Granit  (73).]  E.  Gecko 
eye  illuminated  with  an  intensity  of  1J50  meter -candles;  the  first  record  represents  3.8  sec.  illumi- 
nation after  i  min.  in  the  dark,  the  second  record  2.8  sec.  illumination  after  2  min.  in  the  dark 
(time  marks  0.2  sec.  apart).  [From  Dodt  &  Heck  (47).]  In  records,  A  to  E  illumination  periods 
are  indicated  by  shift  in  the  signal  line. 


for  many  types  of  eyes  in  different  conditions.  The 
reviews  referred  to  should  be  consulted,  supplemented 
with  recent  work  (i  1 1,  114). 

The  problems  of  electroretinography  have  centered 
around  the  following  main  issues;  a)  differentiation 
of  rod  and  cone  ERG's,  b')  analysis  of  the  transition 
from  rod  to  cone  dominance  in  mixed  retinae,  c)  at- 
tempts to  split  the  ERG  into  component  responses, 
d')  comparisons  between  the  ERG  and  the  discharge 
in  the  optic  nerve  and  e")  attempts  to  localize  com- 
ponents of  the  ERG  to  specific  structures  in  the 
retina.  Under  this  heading  also  fall  the  recent  experi- 
ments with  penetrating  capillary  microelectrodes  of 
the  Gerard-Ling  type.  Finally  as  /)  should  be  men- 
tioned a  steadily  expanding  literature  on  electroretin- 
ography in  man  from  descriptive,  theoretical  and 
clinical  points  of  view.  All  these  aspects  cannot  be 
discu.sscd  with  full  attention  to  detail.  Leading  refer- 
ences will,  however,  be  given  within  all  of  them. 

The  pure  cone  ERG's  illustrated  in  figure  4  (.4  and 
B)  are  from  the  horned  toad  (33)  and  a  squirrel  (8) 
and  are  essentially  alike.  These  types  were  called 
Trctinograms  (69),  as  long  as  there  were  no  pure  cone 
mammalian  ERG's  available.  The  cone  eyes  of 
squirrel  species  (8,  g,  25)  have  since  provided  the 
evidence  necessary  for  identifying  the  I-type  with  the 
cone  ERG  in  mammals  as  well  as  other  animals. 
There  is  no  secondary  rise  or  c-wave.  Cone  ERG's  are 
often  negative  in  between  the  h-  and  c/-waves  (cf. 
fig.  4.-I).  They  also  tend  to  have  large  a-waves.  The 
rod  ERG's  (C  and  Z))  are  from  guinea  pig  and  cat 
eyes,  the  former  having  a  practically  pure  rod  retina, 
the  latter  with  cones  corresponding  roughly  to  the 
number  found  in  the  human  peripheral  retina.  The 
ERG  labeled  E  is  from  the  pure  rod  retina  of  the 
gecko  (47).  It  diflfers  from  that  of  the  guinea  pig  in 
showing  a  (/-wave  at  'off.'  Now  guinea  pigs  afso  have 
off-di.scharges  in  their  optic  nerves  (a  very  striking 
feature  of  cone  eyes)  and  so  the  absence  of  the  corre- 
sponding (-/-wave  in  their  ERG  suggests  that  elements 
responding  at  'off'  are  fewer  in  number  than  in  cone 
eyes  and  many  other  types  of  rod  eyes  (cf.  69).  In  the 
ERG  of  the  cat  (C),  for  instance,  the  d-wavc  is  re- 
duced to  a  plateau,  or  a  retardation  at  'off'  in  the 
drop  of  potential  towards  the  base  line.  Walls  (146) 
holds  the  rods  of  the  gecko  to  be  transmuted  cones. 
The  a-wave  seems  to  occur  in  all  retinae,  provided 
the  light  intensity  is  sufficiently  high  to  elicit  it. 

By  changing  state  of  adaptation  from  scotopic  to 
photopic  it  is  also  possible  to  demonstrate  in  mixed 
eyes  that  the  on-off  differentials,  the  a-  and  rf-waves, 
become    faster,    the    rf-wave    in    addition    Ijecoming 


NEUR.I1L    .ACTIVITY    IN    THE    RETINA  699 

Dog  Rabbit 


Fio.  5.  A  comparison  of  the  electroretinogram  of  the  red 
Irish  setter  and  tlie  rabbit.  The  upper  tracing  in  each  record 
gives  the  electroretinogram  and  the  lower,  the  time  mark. 
The  Ught  is  'on'  when  the  time  tracing  is  displaced  upwards. 
The  record  shows  that  the  normal  positive  c-wave  of  rabbits 
is  replaced  by  a  negative  potential  in  dogs.  Time  mark:  0.5  sec. 
Calibration:  50  ^v.  [From  Parry  el  al.  (118).] 


larger  than  before  (41,  46,  80,  113).  This  is  conven- 
iently done  by  using  flickering  light.  Rod  ERG's 
tend  to  flicker  with  repeated  positi\e  i-waves,  while 
in  cone  ERG's  a-waves  and  rZ-waves  (see  below)  also 
take  part  in  the  response  to  intermittent  illumination. 

By  varying  stimulus  intensity  from  the  threshold 
upwards  it  can  easily  be  shown  that  both  the  b-  and 
the  rf-waves  consist  of  several  components  of  different 
rates  of  rise  and  of  different  size  though  complete  sep- 
aration may  be  diflicult  to  achieve.  Systematic  atten- 
tion to  this  problem  was  first  given  in  two  papers  on 
the  frog  eye  (78,  84)  and,  by  varying  state  of  adapta- 
tion and  wavelengths,  the  authors  could  show  that 
some  components  belonged  to  rods  (slow  ones)  and 
some  to  cones.  Similar  differentiations  for  the  human 
eye  were  followed  by  Motokawa  &  Mita  (iio)  and 
Adrian  (i,  2).  Variations  in  duration  (149)  and  in 
rate  of  rise  of  the  stimulating  light  (126)  have  recently 
been  found  to  be  convenient  methods  of  separating 
scotopic  and  photopic  components.  (This  matter  is 
further  considered  below.) 

Deteriorating  ERG's  tend  to  become  cornea-nega- 
tive, but  in  many  animals  the  negative  phase  is  also  a 
normal  feature  of  the  response  and  then  is  always 
found  to  succeed  the  cornea-positive  A-wave  (see  fig. 
4.4).  In  fact,  a  large  negative  phase  occurs  fairly 
generally  in  high  intensity  ERG's  and,  after  light- 
adaptation,  also  in  rod  eyes.  Figure  5  illustrates  the 
ERG  of  a  dog  (118)  which  obviously  contains  a  slow 
negative  component  not  visible  in  the  ERG  of  the 
rabbit  inserted  for  comparison.  Noell's  (114)  work  is 
of  particular  interest  from  this  point  of  view,  as  has 
been  discussed  by  Granit  (73). 

The  common  occurrence  of  slow  or  semistationary 
negative   phases   during    illumination    has    led    most 


700  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^'  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


III 


FIG.  6.   Components  of  the  cat  ERG:   PI,   PII  and  PHI.  The  two  alternatives  for  PII  drawn 
on  the  basis  of  experimental  results.   [From  Granit  (64).] 


workers  to  assume  that  the  ERG  is  an  algebraic  sum 
of  component  processes  of  opposite  signs.  The  methods 
used  for  analysis  have  been  based  on  variations  of 
stimulus  intensity,  duration  and  state  of  adaptation 
as  well  as  on  direct  interference  with  the  ERG  by 
chemical  agents  and  asph\xia.  For  an  orientation  in 
this  field  the  reader  is  referred  to  the  detailed  discus- 
sion by  Granit  (69)  supplemented  by  more  recent 
work  (7,  III,  114).  The  three  components  of  the 
author's  analysis  (fig.  6)  are  based  on  many  observa- 
tions in  the  classical  literature  (quoted  in  the  intro- 
duction) and  certain  of  his  own  experiments  (64,  80) 
and  have  served  for  some  time  now  as  a  summary  and 
a  starting  point  for  further  work. 

There  is  general  agreement  about  the  existence  of  a 
slow  cornea-positive  component  such  as  PI  which  is 
responsiiile  for  the  secondary  rise  or  (-wave  (see  also 
114,  144).  This  requires  fairly  high  intensity  and  not 
too  short  exposures.  There  is  much  evidence  to  show 
that  the  cornea-negative  a-wave  is  the  first  .sign  of 
illumination  (cf.  3,  34,  69)  and  that  it  passes  over  into 
a  slower  negative  phase  which  is  often  submerged 
below  a  mainly  cornea-positive  rcspon.se  but  is  some- 
times visible.  The  component  PHI  appears  to  survive 
damage  to  the  retina  better  than  the  other  ones.  Noell 
(114)  uses  poisoning  with  iodate  to  produce  it  in  the 
rabbit's  eye.  It  has  been  suggested  (69,  73)  that  the 
negative  PHI  consists  of  two  components,  one  fast 
and  the  other  slow.  This  view  has  been  elaisorated  in 
consideraijle  detail  by  Noell  (114),  particularly  with 
regard  to  the  slow  phase. 

A  con\cnient  way  of  making  the  retina  respond 
quickly  to  illumination  by  a  fairly  pure  negative  ERG 
is  to  drop  potassium  chloride  solution  into  the  opened 
bulb  (83,  139).  This  is  a  well-known  depolarizing 
agent  and  accordingly  the  remaining  cornea-negative 
response  to  light  cannot  itself  be  a  depolarization  of 
already  depolarized  structures.  The  cornea-positive 
PII,  however,  is  likely  to  represent  depolarization  by 
light.  Both  components  are  increased  by  running  a 
polarizing  current  across  the  bulb,  inside  negative, 
and  are  decreased  by  reversal  of  this  current  (18,  28, 


76).  A  negative  ERG  can  be  made  positive  Ijy  drop- 
ping alcohol  into  the  bulb  (19). 

At  cessation  of  illumination  PHI  returns  towards 
the  base  line  of  the  record  (the  .so-called  resting  po- 
tential discussed  below),  first  rapidly,  then  more 
slowly.  At  least  in  the  isolated  state  the  slow  returning 
phase  may  appear  as  a  kind  of  'remnant  negativity.' 
[There  are  apparently  still  slower  changes  of  poten- 
tial, both  negative  and  po-sitive  (see  114),  than  the 
ones  generally  counted  as  belonging  to  the  ERG 
proper.]  At  the  same  time  the  cornea-positive  PII 
ends  at  cessation  of  illumination,  either  by  returning 
to  the  base  line  or  even  going  below  it  or  else  contrib- 
uting to  the  rf-wave  that  otherwise  would  have  been 
due  merely  to  interference  between  PII  and  PHI. 
There  is  evidence  for  both  alternatives  in  the  litera- 
ture according  to  the  view  of  Granit  (73).  Further 
experimentation  with  different  eyes  seems  necessary 
to  establish  the  dominant  event  in  difierent  types  of 
eye  (see  6,  82,  113,  141). 

In  considering  questions  of  this  kind  it  is  necessary 
never  to  forget  that  the  ERG  is  a  mass  response  re- 
corded at  a  distance  from  the  sources  generating  its 
potential.  VVirth  &  Zcttcrstrom  (150)  illuminated  the 
cat's  eye  through  perspex  cones  applied  directly  onto 
the  retina  and  found  that  illumination  of  an  area  of 
20  mm-  was  necessary  for  maximal  responses.  Con- 
sidering that  the  diameter  of  the  rods  is  0.D02  mm, 
there  is  ample  margin  for  a  large  variety  of  elementary 
component  responses  to  complicate  the  issue.  A  gen- 
eral analysis  can  merely  aim  at  describing  dominant 
features.  Localized  leads  and  localized  light  projec- 
tions on  the  retina  are  necessary  for  a  study  of  details. 
If  one  illuminates  through  a  glass  electrode  applied 
directly  onto  the  retina  (20),  the  individual  retino- 
grams  are  very  different  in  different  places. 

There  are  a  number  of  interesting  features  by  which 
the  cornea-positive  PII  and  the  cornea-negative  PHI 
of  the  general  analysis  differ  from  one  another.  Figure 
7.-I,  which  illustrates  for  the  frog  retina  the  effect  of 
reilluminating  at  different  times  after  cessation  of 
illumination  (46,   80,    112),   shows  that   the  cornea- 


NEURAL    ACTIVITY    IN    THE    RETINA 


701 


negative  a-wave  now  is  greatly  increased  and  maximal 
when  the  off-effect  has  reached  full  size.  The  retina 
seems  to  show  no  refractoriness  but  is  immediately 
ready  to  re-establish  the  level  of  negativity  charac- 
teristic of  that  particular  state  of  adaptation  and 
stimulus  strength.  The  cornea-positive  PI  I  (6-wave) 
behaves  very  differently.  It  fails  to  appear  until  some 
time  has  passed,  as  can  also  be  very  clearly  seen  with 
the  cat  retina  (Z))  which  is  dominated  by  this  com- 
ponent. 

Figure  8  shows  the  full  analysis  of  an  experiment  of 
this  kind.  The  dotted  lines  represent  the  effects  of  the 
individual  flashes  of  reillumination,  d  is  the  rf-wave 
control;  h,  the  level  of  the  A-waves;  a,  that  of  the  a- 
waves.  Assuming  a  and  b  being  generated  in  the  same 
structures,  it  is  difficult  to  understand  why  the  former 
response  is  immediately  ready  to  be  re-established 
while  the  latter  refuses  to  behave  in  the  same  fashion. 
Part  of  this  difference  has  been  found  to  be  due  to 
the  b-  and  ^/-waves  sharing  generators  in  the  sense 
that  the  one  leaves  refractoriness  for  the  other  (82). 

In  recent  attempts  to  assign  the  origin  of  the  ERG 
to  definite  retinal  structures,  experiments  of  this  type 
have  been  neglected  altogether.  Yet,  they  seem  to 
contain  essential  information  about  the  components 
of  the  ERG  which  no  discussion  of  these  problems  can 


neglect.  Perhaps  this  is  the  place  for  pointing  out  that 
the  ganglion  cells  definitely  seem  to  be  excluded  as 
sources  of  the  ERG.  From  time  to  time  since  1933  the 
author  has  stimulated  antidromically  the  optic  nerve 
of  frogs  and  cats  while  recording  the  ERG  in  order 


FIG.  7.  Effect  of  increasing  the  interval  between  two  stimuli 
on  the  electroretinogram  of  different  types  of  retinae:  .4,  frog; 
D,  cat.  Uppermost  curve  of  each  series  shows  the  uninterrupted 
off-effect.  Short  vertical  lines  indicate  the  beginning  of  re- 
illumination.  Time  marking:  o.i   sec.   [From  Granit  (65).] 


U  U-l  0-2 


FIG.  8.  Off-effect  or  (/-wave  given  by  d  in  the  frog  ERG.  Reillumination  by  single  flashes  elicits 
the  potential  changes  (a-  and  i-waves)  shown  in  dotted  lines,  a  and  b  trace  maxima  of  a-  and  i-waves 
respectively.  Note  that  curves  b  and  a  are  drawn  through  peaks  of  b  and  a  waves  at  different 
times  of  re-illumination  and  thus  show  that  the  a-wave  reappears  at  once  and  is  increased  while 
the  4-wavc  requires  a  long  time  for  recovery.  [From  Granit  c&  Riddell  (80).] 


702 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


400r 


300- 


200 


e"ioo 


O  O 


X 


0 


200 


400 


600 


800  0- 


FIG.  g.  Effect  of  a  Hash,  supcrimposfd  on  the  off-discharge 
in  the  frog's  optic  nerve,  plotted  as  average  spike  frequency 
in  impulses  per  sec.  against  time  in  msec.  Dotted  line,  off- 
discharge  control.  Continuous  line,  diminution  in  frequency 
(inhibition)  caused  by  a  flash,  indicated  by  the  horizontal  black 
line,  delivered  at  the  height  of  the  off-effect.  [From  Granit  & 
Therman  (8i).] 


to  determine  whether  the  latter  could  be  influenced. 
This  has  never  been  the  case  (76).  Yet  it  is  possible  to 
influence  both  the  discharge  of  ganglion  cells  and 
their  level  of  depolarization  by  such  means  (74). 
Further,  glaucoma  with  optic  nerve  atrophy  is  asso- 
ciated with  a  normal  ERG  (96,  114). 

The  question  of  how  the  ERG  correlates  with  the 
discharge  through  the  optic  nerve  can  be  profitably 
attacked  by  studying  what  happens  in  the  nerve  when 
these  large  a-waves  are  induced  on  top  of  the  ofT- 
discharge.  Figure  9  shows  that  there  is  inhibition  of 
the  off-discharge  (81)  and  this  was  confirmed  by 
Granit  &  Helme  (76).  This  experiment  provided  the 
main  argument  for  the  view  that  PHI  is  concerned 
with  inhibition  rather  than  with  excitation.  The  cor- 
nea-positive PII  was  held  to  signify  excitation.  The 
general  likeness  between  the  cornea-positive  retino- 
gram  and  the  variation  of  the  discharge-frequency 
through  the  optic  nerve,  first  pointed  out  lay  Adrian 
&  Matthews  (3,  4,  5),  supports  the  same  inference 
(fig.  10).  Granit  &  Therman  (81)  could  find  no  eff"ect 
in  the  optic  nerve  discharge  corresponding  to  the 
large  c-wave  of  the  dark  adapted  eye. 

In  this  connection  the  effect  of  alcohol  on  the  frog 
retina  is  particularly  interesting  (19).  The  left  record 
in  figure  1 1  shows  the  light  adapted  ERG  of  a  frog. 
A  drop  of  alcohol  into  the  opened  bulb  makes  it 
change  as  shown  in  the  right  record.  The  upper 
record  shows  the  change  of  the  a-wave  under  alcohol. 
In  this  final  stage  it  looks  like  the  ERG  of  the  fully 
dark  adapted  retina  and,  like  the  latter,  has  a  small 
d-wavc  and  responds  only  to  slowly  flickering  light. 
Even  an  ERG  made  largely  negative  by  potassium 


200- 


FiG.    10.    Variation   in   impulse   frequency   of  the   eel   optic 
nerve  during  stimulation.   [From  Adrian  &   Matthews  (3).] 


1 


^ 


FTP 


FIG.  II.  Effect  of  alcohol  on  frog  ERG.  Top  record:  diminu- 
tion of  a-wave  during  alcohol  treatment.  Left:  Light-adapted 
ERG  with  reillumination  on  top  of  the  rf-wave.  Right:  Same 
experiment  after  alcohol.  Time,  0.2  sec.  Light  signal  above 
time  record.    [From   Bernhard   &   Skoglund  (ig).] 


chloride  turns  positive  after  alcohol.  The  interpre- 
tation of  these  changes  is  that  alcohol  diminishes  PHI 
and  augments  PII,  which  agrees  with  the  interpreta- 
tion of  the  similar  changes  with  state  of  adaptation. 
However,  still  more  interesting  from  the  present  point 
of  view  is  what  happens  in  the  test  with  reillumination 
in  which,  after  alcohol,  the  negative  a-wave  will  now 
be  very  small  or  absent.  The  experiment  showed  that 
the  corresponding  inhibitory  pau.se  in  the  discharge 
also  was  curtailed.  The  authors  held  their  result  to 
support  the  view  that  PHI  (or  what  now  may  be 
called  fast  PHI)  was  inhil)itory,  i.e.  as  confirming 
the  view  that  excitation  and  inhibition  was  charac- 
terized ijy  opposite  deflections  of  the  ERG. 

In  this  volume  the  vision  of  invertebrates  is  treated 
in  Chapter  XX\"I  by  Milne  and  Milne.  It  is  neverthe- 
less of  considerable  interest  here  to  mention  the  recent 
experiments  of  MacNichol  &  Benolken  (105)  with 
Hartline's  so-called  lateral  inhiljition  in  the  plexus  of 
nerve  fibers  behind  the  Limulus  ommatidia  (9O.  By 
means  of  interconnections  in  this  plexus,  illumination 
of  one  ommatidium  suppresses  the  discharge  of 
another  (91).  Now,  MacNichol  cS:  Benolken  find  that 
alcohol  removes  this  lateral  inhibition  re\ersil)ly. 

For  a  final  allocation  of  the  retinal  component  po- 
tentials  to   definite   structures  (receptors  or   bipolar 


NEURAL  ACTIVITY   IN   THE  RETINA 


703 


cells),  it  will  be  necessary  to  use  nonpolarizable  pene- 
trating microelectrodes.  Such  work  was  initiated  by 
Tomita  (141,  142).  Instead  of  a  review  of  arguments 
from  general  electroretinographic  work  for  which  the 
author's  summary  (69)  may  be  consulted,  a  brief  dis- 
cussion of  the  results  obtained  by  such  niicromcthods 
is  given. 

Tomita"s  work  was  criticized  and  ignored  by  Otto- 
son  &  Svaetichin  (i  16)  on  the  grounds  that  his  pene- 
trating microelectrodes  were  held  to  be  too  coarse. 
Tomita  has  since  (142)  repeated  his  experiments  with 
the  Ling-Gerard  type  of  microcapillaries,  used  by 
Svaetichin,  and  confirmed  his  previous  results.  At  the 
same  time  Brindley  (28,  29,  30)  has  also  published  a 
careful  study  using  the  same  technique.  Ottoson  & 
Svaetichin's  conclusions  as  far  as  fundamental  ques- 
tions are  concerned  were:  a)  that  slow  potentials  of 
the  ERG  type  are  obtained  in  the  receptors  only; 
this  has  since  been  definitely  refuted  by  Brindley  (30) 
and  by  Tomita  &  Torihama  (142),  who  found  large 
potential  changes  mirroring  the  ERG  in  the  bipolar 
layer;  b)  that  rods  respond  only  with  positive,  and 
cones  only  with  negative  retinograms  [cf.  also  Svaeti- 
chin (137)],  disproved  by  Granit  (73)  as  well  as  by 
Forbes  ft  al.  (55)  and  Brindley  (29);  c)  that  the  resting 
potential  of  the  retina  is  a  receptor  potential  (115), 
since  refuted  by  Brindley  (28);  tt)  that,  from  the  fact 
that  cocaine  slowly  attacks  the  ERG  but  immediately 
stops  the  discharge  of  impulses  through  the  optic 
nerve,  it  is  possible  to  conclude  that  the  ERG  is  a  pure 
receptor  potential  [from  Kiihne  &  Steiner  (102) 
onwards  the  number  of  agents  capable  of  blocking  the 
impulse  discharge  without  much  effect  on  the  ERG 
has  been  slowly  multiplying,  yet  without  suggesting 
to  anyone  such  far-reaching  conclusions] ;  e)  that  from 
the  size  of  sudden  potential  gradients  of  the  order  of 
20  to  30  mv  within  the  fish  retina  (approached  from 
the  receptor  end)  it  is  po.ssible  to  assume  that  the 
electrode  recorded  intracellularly  from  single  cones. 
Now  Brindley  (28)  has  shown  that  there  are  charac- 
teristic steps  in  the  radial  resistance  to  a  penetrating 
microelectrode,  the  largest  one  across  the  external 
limiting  membrane  (see  below),  and  extracellular 
spike  potentials  of  the  order  of  40  to  60  mv  have  been 
recorded  by  Granit  &  Phillips  (79)  by  the  same  tech- 
nique at  the  surface  of  the  cerebellar  Purkinje  cells. 
Furthermore,  an  extracellular  retinal  microelectrode 
has  been  shown  by  Brindley  (30)  and  Tomita  &  Tori- 
hama (142)  to  pick  up  its  response  from  very  distant 
illuminated  regions.  This  is,  of  course,  what  one  must 
expect.  Light  intensities  and  techniques  of  illumina- 
tion  are   hardly   ever   mentioned   in   the   papers   by 


Svaetichin  &  Ottoson  but  this,  in  itself,  suggests  that 
the  whole  retina  or  a  large  fraction  of  it  was  illumi- 
nated. Focal  microillumination  would  be  needed  for 
localized  responses  and  there  is  in  its  favor  the  further 
advantage  that  absence  of  a  response  within  the  bi- 
polar layer  shows  whether  damage  has  occurred  (30, 
142).  Such  damage  probably  explains  why  Ottoson 
&  Svaetichin  have  missed  the  response  inside  the 
retina.  In  fact,  Brindley  (30)  describes  two  types  of 
responses  in  excised  frog  eyes  of  which  the  .second 
agrees  with  the  pictures  of  Svaetichin.  This  type 
Brindley  holds  to  be  characteristic  of  local  damage 
because  it  alone  is  seen  when  the  focal  intraretinal 
response  is  absent.  These  remarks  may  suffice  to  show 
why  it  is  felt  that  Ottoson  &  Svaetichin  have  under- 
rated the  analytical  difficulties  of  the  work  they  set 
out  to  do.  For  this  reason  individual  good  observa- 
tions in  their  work,  perhaps  unjustly,  lose  their  sig- 
nificance to  a  reviewer  and  can  only  be  rescued  by 
those  who  have  undertaken  microelectrode  work  with 
the  same  structure  and  thus  can  evaluate  them  criti- 
cally against  a  background  of  specific  experience. 

Apparently  retinal  neurons  do  not  differ  from  other 
neurons,  all  of  them  producing  potential  changes. 
Both  Brindley  and  Tomita  have  arranged  their  ex- 
periments for  comparison  of  precise  focal  microil- 
lumination around  the  microelectrode  tip  with  illu- 
mination of  larger  areas.  Unless  an  electrode  within 
the  bipolar  area  responds  to  focal  illumination  with  a 
reponse  of  the  intraretinal  type,  the  region  around 
this  electrode  is  not  likely  to  be  in  a  normal  state. 
Brindley  suggests  that  this  condition  is  due  to  dam- 
age of  the  external  limiting  membrane.  The  inside 
focal  response  has  a  maximum  among  the  bipolar 
cells  at  a  depth  of  1 00  to  1 40  yii  from  the  ganglion  side. 
The  large  ERG  elicited  by  general  illumination  is 
always  obtained  and  tends  to  be  positive.  The  two 
authors  differ  in  that  Tomita's  response  to  diffuse 
light  reverses  sign  within  the  inner  nuclear  layer, 
going  from  positive  at  the  ganglion  side  to  negative 
at  the  receptor  side  of  the  retina,  while  Brindley  finds 
more  variability  in  this  regard.  Now,  is  the  focal 
response  inside  the  retina  identical  with  the  ERG? 
Obviously  this  response  is  physiologically  important 
but  both  Brindley  and  Tomita  argue  against  identi- 
fication. In  the  reviewer's  opinion  it  is  impossible  at 
the  present  stage  of  our  knowledge  to  be  certain  as  to 
whether  or  not  bipolar  cells  contribute  to  the  ERG. 
Tomita,  for  instance,  finds  no  focal  response  within 
the  receptor  layer.  This  is  no  crucial  objection  to  a 
localization  in  the  receptors,  nevertheless  it  is  a  fact 
to  be  considered.  Brindley  holds  the  focal  response  to 


704 


HANDBOOK    OF    I'HYSKJLOGV 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


be  caused  by  sources  located  tangentially  within  the 
retina  and  suggests,  on  the  basis  of  electrical  con- 
siderations too  involved  to  discuss,  that  "only  the 
rods  and  cones  contribute  substantially  to  the  a-,  b- 
and  (/-waves  of  the  electroretinogram,  although  cells 
belonging  to  the  inner  nuclear  layer  can  produce 
large  electrical  changes  of  similar  time  course"  (30). 
He  himself  finds  this  hypothesis  "somewhat  surpris- 
ing." Tomita  localizes  the  same  response  to  the 
bipolar  layer  with  some  contributions  from  other 
retinal  layers,  in  particular  the  inner  portion  of  the 
receptors   [cf.    also   Noell   (114)]. 

Particularly  interesting  are  Brindley's  (28)  meas- 
urements of  the  passive  electrical  properties  of  frog 
retina  for  radial  fields  and  currents  and  the  method 
applied  to  make  such  measurements  possible.  The 
largest  step  takes  place  at  around  230  fx  from  the 
ganglion  side  and  is  of  the  order  of  270  fi.  This  is 
Brindley's  R-membrane  which  he  pro\isionally  identi- 
fies with  the  external  limiting  membrane.  Its  capacity 
is  about  40  /i  F.  Across  this  memi^rane  also  is  the  largest 
step  or  component  of  the  resting  potential  of  the 
retina.  Electron  microscopy  (134,  135)  has  led  to  the 
view  that  the  external  limiting  membrane  consists 
of  rings  or  collars  around  the  receptor  base  (inner 
segment),  a  fact  difficult  to  harmonize  with  the  high 
resistance  and  capacity  if  it  really  can  be  assumed 
that  there  has  been  no  shrinkage  in  Sjostrand's 
preparations. 

These  papers  are  thought-provoking  and  serious 
attempts  to  lay  bare  the  considerable  difficulties  in 
arriving  at  evidence  for  final  conclusions  and  thus 
form  a  structure  of  knowledge  upon  which  further 
work  can  be  built.  There  is  a  large  body  of  informa- 
tion about  the  ERG  as  influenced  by  alcohol,  potas- 
sium, state  of  adaptation,  etc.,  which  will  have  to  be 
experimentally  applied  to  microelectrode  analysis  be- 
fore a  final  conclusion  can  be  reached. 

The  crucial  point  in  the  present  position  is  the 
identification  of  the  R-membrane,  it  being  highly 
unlikely  that  any  intracellular  potentials  in  the  retina 
ever  have  been  recorded.  The  recent  findings  by 
McNichol  et  al.  {105a)  show  that  the  former  cone- 
potential  of  Svaetichin  actually  is  obtained  below  the 
layer  of  rods  and  cones  and  Tomita  (141a)  has  shown 
that  it  can  be  obtained  with  electrodes  far  too  big  to 
penetrate  individual  cells  successfully.  However,  the 
observations  on  effects  of  different  wavelengths  in  fish 
seem  interesting  independently  of  present  assumptions 


m 


oft 


11 


pJ    I    I    I 


oft 

— I 


.d_L 


.1   I   I   llllllllllllll  I  II  Mill II  I  I   I   I   I 

FIG.  12.  Diagiain  illustrating  tliree  libers  in  the  optic  nerve 
firing  spontaneously  and  their  responses  to  illumination  as 
described  in  text.   [From  Granit  (73).] 


and   also  confirm   the  demonstration  of  dominators 
and  narrow-banded  modulators  in  this  eye  (65a). 


NEUR.\L    P.^TTERNS 

Work  on  the  spike  discharge  from  the  retina  started 
with  the  classical  papers  by  Adrian  &  Matthews 
(3,  4,  5)  who  used  the  long  optic  nerve  of  the  eel 
Conger  for  a  study  of  the  massed  discharge.  The  dis- 
covery of  different  discharge  types  was  a  consequence 
of  Hartline's  (86)  successful  attempts  to  split  the 
frog  optic  nerve.  This  he  did  at  the  point  where  it 
enters  the  blind  spot  and  is  already  naturally  split 
into  fibers  coming  from  different  parts  of  the  retina. 
The  types  are  shown  in  the  schematic  figure  12  in 
which  account  also  is  taken  of  the  microelectrode 
records  from  mammals  (see  69,  73)  in  which  one 
often  finds  more  activity  between  onset  and  cessation 
of  light  than  in  the  frog  and  less  stability  of  response 
types.  Some  fibers  (/  in  fig.  12)  respond  to  onset  of 
light,  others  (2)  are  inhibited  by  onset  of  light  and 
instead  discharge  at  'ofT."  The  majorit\-  of  them  dis- 
charge to  both  onset  and  cessation  of  light  (j).  Re- 
illumination  during  the  off-discharge  inhibits  it,  as 
shown  by  2h.  It  should  be  realized  that,  since  the 
optic  nerve  fibers  represent  highly  differentiated  con- 
vergence structures,  there  are  in  actual  practice  al- 
most as  many  discharge  types  as  there  are  optic  nerve 
fibers.  Nevertheless  the  types  illustrated  show  ap- 
proximately what  happens.  The  inhibition  of  the 
ofT-discharge,  as  stated,  coincides  with  the  large 
negative   o-wave   on   top   of  the   off-effect   described 


NEURAL   ACTIVITY   IN    THE   RETINA 


705 


above.  In  most  eyes  there  is  some  spontaneous  firing, 
generally  greater  in  the  scotopic  state. 

The  salient  point  with  regard  to  the  general  prob- 
lem of  response  types  is,  as  shown  by  Granit  (71,  72) 
and  Kuffler  (100),  that  the  retina  contains  two  an- 
tagonistic systems,  the  on-system  and  the  off-system, 
which,  when  made  to  clash  on  to  the  same  ganglion 
cell,  are  mutually  exclusive.  One  system  is  excited  by 
light  (the  on-system),  the  other  is  inhibited  by  light 
(the  off-system)  and  the  latter  behaves  as  if  the  longer, 
within  limits,  the  duration  during  which  inhibition  is 
piled  up,  the  more  did  this  favor  the  subsequent 
off-discharge.  Thus,  during  the  time  the  off-discharge 
is  inhibited  by  light  something  happens  that  makes 
it  prone  to  respond  when  ultimately  light  is  exchanged 
for  darkness.  Short  exposures  tend  to  give  very  brief 
off-effects.  When  a  definite  off-effect  is  seen  in  the 
ERG,  e.g.  in  frogs,  it  also  behaves  similarly. 

The  anatomical  convergence  means  that  each 
fiber  has  a  receptive  field,  first  measured  by  Adrian 
&  Matthews  (3,  4,  5)  and  shown  to  be  of  the  order  of 
I  mm  in  the  eye  of  the  conger  eel,  then  more  pre- 
cisely with  the  single-fiber  technique  by  Hartline 
(87).  Figure  1 3  shows  the  exploring  spot  used  by 
him  and  the  field  sizes  obtained  in  the  frog  eye  when 
stimulus  strength  was  varied.  Just  as  convergence 
varies  from  fiber  to  fiber,  so  do  the  dimensions  of  the 
receptive  fields.  In  cats  Kuffler  (100)  found  them 
beautifully  organized  so  that  sometimes  the  on-, 
sometimes  the  off-effect  occupied  the  center,  the 
opposite  response  then  occupying  its  periphcrv  and 
on-off-responses  occurring  between  the  two.  This 
provided  Kuffler  with  a  good  opportunity  to  make 
on-  and  off-spots  of  the  receptive  field  clash  in  \arious 
combinations  and  thus  elegantly  to  demonstrate  the 
antagonism  between  the  two  systems  relative  to  the 
ganglion  discharge.  A  very  complete  discussion  of 
receptive  fields  and  on-off  systems  has  been  given  by 
Granit  (73).  Both  principles  of  organization  recur  in 
the  central  structures  to  which  receptors  in  other 
sense  organs  project. 

Why  then  is  the  lining  of  receptors  inside  the  e)e 
connected  to  an  intricate  nervous  center  just  behind 
it,  while  other  receptor  systems  mostlv  have  their 
first  neural  organization  at  the  spinal  cord  level? 
Apparently  receptors  cannot  do  much  by  themselves; 
their  mes.sagcs  must  be  organized  somehow  for  dis- 
crimination and  integration;  and,  since  the  little 
brain  behind  the  rods  and  cones  moves  with  the  eye, 
it  can  because  of  its  place  in  the  retina  aid  better  in 
the  interpretation  of  the  ever-changing  boundaries  of 


FIG.  13.  Chait  of  the  receptive  field  of  a  single  optic  nerve 
fiber  of  the  frog.  Each  fine  encloses  a  retinal  region  within  which 
the  exploring  spot  light  (relative  size  shown  above  left) — the  log 
of  the  intensity  is  given  on  the  line — produced  a  response  from 
the  fiber.  On  each  line  the  indicated  intensity  was  the  threshold; 
the  set  of  curves  constitutes  a  contour  map  of  the  distribution 
of  the  retinal  sensitivity  to  light  with  reference  to  this  par- 
ticular fiber.  [From  Hartline  (89).] 


light,  darkness  and  color  out  of  which  the  visual 
world  is  synthesized.  The  eye,  as  stated  above,  is 
never  still  and,  if  by  artificial  means  the  image  is  kept 
stationary  (39,  125),  it  tends  to  fade  out  quickly, 
as  if  it  needed  the  on-off  differentials  sharpening  up 
contours  during  oscillations.  Movement  of  an  object 
or  a  point  across  the  retina  will  light  up  a  trail  of  on- 
off  sparks,  as  well  shown  by  Barlow  (15,  16)  in  frog 
experiments  set  up  to  illustrate  the  biological  sig- 
nificance of  such  factors  for  perceprion.  Apparently 
also,  it  is  necessary  for  this  highly  developed  organ 
not  to  be  forced  to  one  single  mode  of  working  in 
coping  with  a  range  of  illumination  from  dusk  to 
bright  sunlight.  It  has  been  shown  that  the  receptive 
fields  of  the  retina  vary  in  width  with  state  of  adapta 
tion  (17). 

From  what  has  l)een  stated  it  is  clear  that  at  least 
within  a  receptive  field  interaction  can  occur,  as 
first  shown  by  Adrian  &  Matthews  (5)  and  then 
studied  in  detail  by  Hartline  (87,  88).  When  spike 
frequency  or  latency  is  used  as  an  indicator,  area  and 
intensity  are  found  to  he  interchangeable  within  the 
field,  whether  from  excitation  or  inhibition  being 
unknown.  These  facts  provide  a  likely  explanation 
of  the  many  old  psychophysical  observations  on  the 
interchangeability  of  area  and  intensity  in  vision. 
From  what  has  been  stated  it  will  be  realized  that 
on-off  interaction  adds  to  the  complexity  so  that  over 


7o6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


a  large  range  of  intensities  the  on-off  ratio  will  undergo 
considerable  variation.  This  variation  has  been  held 
to  support  a  mechanism  of  discrimination  based  on 
the  overlapping  receptive  fields.  Overlapping  recep- 
tive fields,  less  elaborated  than  in  the  retina,  also 
occur  in  other  sense  organs.  These  principles  have 
been  discussed  at  some  length  by  Granit  (73). 

Many  problems  of  retinal  neurology  have  been 
clarified  by  Hartline's  work  on  the  much  simpler 
Limulus  eye,  to  which  students  of  the  verteljrate  retina 
are  advised  to  give  careful  attention.  This  in  par- 
ticular applies  to  the  recent  important  analysis  of  the 
lateral  inhibition  (90)  in  the  neural  network  below 
the  ommatidia,  because  the  suppression  of  the  dis- 
charge of  one  ommatidium  by  its  illuminated  neigh- 
bor is  clearly  a  mechanism  of  contrast.  In  the  frog 
retina  Barlow  (15,  16)  has  found  the  discharge  from 
a  receptive  field  to  be  inhibited  by  illuminating  retinal 
regions  just  outside  it. 

There  are  other  aspects  to  the  problem  of  inhibition 
than  those  connected  with  the  organization  ot  the 
receptive  fields.  There  is,  for  example  postexcitatory 
inhibition  (73)  and  the  generalized  inhibition  recently 
described  Ijy  Dodt  (43). 


STIMULUS    CORRELATES 

The  average  electroretinographic  response  is 
roughly  proportional  to  the  logarithm  of  stimulus 
intensity.  However,  since  most  retinae  contain  two 
organs  in  one  (rods  and  cones)  and  the  ERG's  of 
rods  and  cones  do  not  relate  similarly  to  stimulus 
intensity  in  addition  to  differing  in  latency  and  rate 
of  rise  (see  above),  too  much  should  not  be  made  out 
of  this  general  logarithmic  relationship.  It  does,  how- 
ever, suggest  that  the  elementary  generator  potentials 
of  which  the  total  response  is  made  up  tend  to  be 
logarithmically  related  to  stimulus  intensity,  and  this, 
whenever  they  have  been  recorded,  actually  is  the 
case  [cf.  the  discussion  by  Granit  (73)].  In  Limulus 
the  spike  frequency  emanating  from  the  excentric  cell 
of  a  single  ommatidium  is  logarithmically  related  to 
stimulus  intensity  meaning  that  the  level  of  om- 
matidial  generator  potential,  at  least  when  stabilized, 
is  directly  proportional  to  spike  frequency  [the  cor- 
responding relation  for  muscle  receptors  has  been 
described  (97)].  Many  single  fiber  preparations  rep- 
resenting receptive  fields  in  frogs  (86)  and  mammals 
(67)  have  shown  a  general  logarithmic  relation  to 
stimulus  intensity,  superimposed,  as  it  were,  upon 
on-off  complexities.  The  overall  effect  of  an  assembly 


of  cells  is  likely  to  follow  this  rule  which,  of  course,  is 
nothing  but  the  well-known  Fechner  law  looked  at  in 
a  diflferent  way  [for  a  full  discussion,  see  Granit  (73)]. 

The  retina  is  a  detector  of  so-called  visible  light 
which  is  visible  because  photochemical  substances 
within  the  rods  and  cones  absorb  energy  and  trans- 
form it  into  a  form  appropriate  for  stimulation.  Most 
color  theories  have  assumed  that  different  kinds  of 
cones  are  provided  with  photochemical  substances  ad- 
justed for  absorption  of  light  within  different  parts  of 
the  spectrum  but  the  only  substance  known  until 
fairly  recently  was  Boll's  visual  purple  or  rhodopsin 
in  the  rod  outer  limbs,  and  these  organs,  on  the 
duplicity  theory,  were  assumed  to  be  color  blind. 
This  was  the  situation  until  some  experiments  with 
the  electroretinogram  (78,  84)  definitely  proved  that 
the  frog  light-adapted  eye  showed  distributions  of 
spectral  sensitivity  that  required  a  minimum  of  three 
cone  substances  to  be  intelligible.  This  has  since  been 
confirmed  in  many  other  types  of  experiments  with 
different  eyes.  In  light-adapted  frogs  and  in  the  cone 
eyes  of  the  turtle,  Forbes  et  al.  (55)  showed  that,  if 
they  were  illuminated  by  two  'white'  lights,  these 
could  be  exchanged  without  influencing  the  ERG  but 
that  certain  pairs  of  colored  lights  never  could  be 
exchanged,  whatever  their  intensity  ratio,  without  a 
specific  electroretinographic  color  response.  Japanese 
workers  (62,  63,  140)  using  frogs  studied  the  multiple 
off-effects  and  wavelets  on  top  of  the  off-effect,  men- 
tioned above,  and  found  evidence  for  a  representation 
of  differential  spectral  sensitivity  in  the  different  crest 
times  of  such  wavelets.  There  was  a  minimum  of  three 
humps  appearing  in  the  order  red,  green  and  blue, 
as  also  shown  in  the  recent  work  of  Heck  &  Rendahl 
(93).  A  similar  order  had  previously  been  observed 
bv  Donner  (51)  working  with  the  spike  frequency- 
time  differentials  of  single  ganglion  cells  in  the  cat 
retina  and  by  Motokawa  and  his  group  (108,  109, 
143).  There  is  a  critical  review  of  Motokawas  work 
by  Gebhard  (59).  Motokawa's  measurements  were 
based  on  the  rate  of  rise  of  retinal  sensitivity  to  a 
brief  polarizing  current  after  preillumination  with 
different  wavelengths. 

Recent  work  with  the  human  electroretinogram  is 
definite  in  showing  that  the  ERG  contains  com- 
ponents of  different  color  sensitivity,  even  in  the  light- 
adapted  state  (10,  II,  93,  130).  HowcNcr,  all  work 
with  a  mass  response  such  as  the  ERG  suff"ers  from 
the  difficulty  of  isolating  the  spectral  components  in  a 
quantitative  way. 

Color  vision  as  an  electrophysiological  problem 
contains  two  different  aspects:  a)  the  primary  sensi- 


NEURAL    ACTIVITY   IN    THE    RETINA 


707 


tivity  distributions  of  individual  receptors  and  b)  their 
representation  in  the  organized  message  dehvered  by 
the  ganglion  cells  through  the  optic  nerve.  The  latter 
delivers  the  information  which  the  striate  area  has  to 
interpret  and  so  problem  b  is  as  important  as  prob- 
lem a.  The  act  of  interpretation  itself  is  at  the  moment 
beyond  the  reach  of  electrophysiological  approach. 
Solution  of  the  first  problem  requires  more  reliable 
microelectrode  records  of  individual  receptor  po- 
tentials than  is  found  in  any  paper  hitherto  presented. 
Our  most  definite  quantitative  data  are  still  the  ones 
obtained  from  individual  optic  nerve  fibers  of  animals 
(66,  68,  6g,  73).  It  is  necessary  to  understand  how  a 
spectral  distribution  is  defined  in  order  to  comprehend 
the  color  problem.  A  simplified  presentation  of  this 
question  can  be  given  in  the  following  way. 

Assume  that  a  color-sensitive  substance  absorbs  light 
along  a  spectral  distribution  curve  represented  in 
every  wavelength  by  S\.  Dependent  upon  the  lamp 
used  and  upon  other  properties  of  the  spectrum  (e.g. 
slit  width,  diffraction)  each  of  these  wavelengths  tested 
represents  an  amount  of  energy  E\.  Finally,  again, 
each  of  the  wavelengths  tested  elicits  an  effect  Lx.  It 
is  immaterial  now  if  this  effect  is  considered  in  terms 
of  a  receptor  potential,  a  spike  frequency  or  as  per- 
ceived brightness.  This  efTect  L>,  will  be  proportional 
to  the  sensitivity  Sx  and  the  amount  of  energy  Ex  so 
that  Lx  =  Ex-Sx.  In  order  to  measure  Sx  which  is  the 
function  we  want  to  study  and  which  clearly  is  .Sx 
=  Lx/Ex  we  must  first  of  all  measure  Ex  of  the  spec- 
trum used  (which  should  be  of  a  high  degree  of 
purity).  The  next  step  is  to  set  up  the  biological  ex- 
periment so  that  the  physiological  effect  Lx  is  kept 
constant  in  every  wavelength.  Then,  with  Lx  and 
Ex  known,  the  equation  can  be  solved,  i.e.  Sx  can  be 
calculated.  It  is  proportional  to  i/Ex.  For  c|u;)ntita- 
tivc  work  it  is  therefore  not  enough  to  keep  the  energy 
E  of  the  spectrum  constant  and  measure  the  physio- 
logical effect  L,  even  though  such  results  may  have 
indicative  value  and  can  be  approximately  corrected  if 
the  relation  between  E  and  L  initially  has  been  meas- 
ured over  the  working  range  for  each  wavelength. 
Very  serious  errors  can  also  be  introduced  by  filters 
of  which  even  the  best  have  narrow  color  bands  over 
one  or  two  log  units  only.  Therefore  spectra  should 
be  used  for  quantitati\e  work.  A  good  method  is  to 
mea.sure  i/Ex  for  a  constant  response  (Lx)  such  as 
the  threshold.  This  was  the  method  employed  in  the 
experiments  on  the  discharge  from  indi\idunl  nerve 
fibers. 

These  results,  for  which  a  large  number  of  different 
species  of  animals  were  used,  some  with   pure  cone 


% 

80 

/ 

'\ 



/ 

\ 

y 

■10 

I 

7 

\ 

?n 

/ 

\ 

> 

^ 

0  450 


0  500 


0-550 


mO  700 


FIG.   14.   Photopic  dominator  curves  of  the  frog  ( )  and 

the  snake  Tropidonotus  natrix  (• •).  Equal  quantum  in- 
tensity spectrum.  Sensitivity  plotted  against  wavelength. 
[From  Granit  (66).] 


retinae,  others  with  mixed  retinae  (after  light- 
adaptation),  led  to  the  dominator-modulator  con- 
cept. The  optic  nerve  fibers  were  found  to  deliver  two 
types  of  curves,  broad-band  dominators  and  narrow- 
band modulators.  Figure  14  shows  photopic  domina- 
tors of  the  snake  cone  eye  and  the  light-adapted  frog 
eye  which  are  of  interest  because  the  photochemical 
systems  of  these  eyes  seem  to  he  very  similar  to  our 
own.  Actually  their  photopic  dominators  agree  very 
\\ell  with  the  average  photopic  distribution  of  sensi- 
tixity  of  the  human  eye.  When  the  mixed  eye  is 
dark-adapted,  the  same  fiber  that  previously  gave  a 
photopic  doiuinator  now  gives  a  scotopic  one  with 

b 

maximum  around  5000  A  which  agrees  with  the 
sensitivity  distriljution  of  visual  purple  or  rhodopsin. 
We  recall  that  Polyak  (122)  had  shown  that  both 
rods  and  cones  converge  towards  the  saiue  ganglion 
cell.  Thus  the  dominators  are  the  carriers  of  the 
Purkinje  shift  of  retinal  sensitivity  with  state  of  adap- 
tation. In  man  the  point  of  maximum  shifts  from 
5560  to  51 00  A,  just  as  in  frogs  and  cats.  The  photo- 
chemical aspects  will  be  discus.sed  elsewhere  in  this 
volume  (Wald,  Chapter  XXVIII),  but  it  deserves  to 
be  pointed  out  that  dominators  in  various  systems 
have  been  synthesized  by  Wald  out  of  vitamin  A 
aldehydes  and  rod  and  cone  proteins  with  the  aid 
of  various  enzymes  and  that  these  synthesized  prod- 
ucts have  absorption  spectra  in  good  agreement  with 
the  experimental  results  obtained  from  optic  nerve 
fibers  [see  also  the  summaries  by  Granit  (73,  75)]. 
Examples  of  modulators  from  different  animals  are 


7o8 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEl'R0PH^■S10L0Cn• 


100 

% 

80 
60 
40 
20 


/ 

■( 

>• 

W/ 

N 

\ 

■ 

■ 

/ 

.A 

'  \j? 

IK  %  j\ 

[\ 

\ 

■ 

f 

■ 

/ 

% 

i.\\ 

• 

■ 

/ 
/ 

r\^'\/ 

\ 

■ ".    1 

\               \ 

1 

V 

■ 

450 


500 


550 


600 


650 


r 


FIG.   15.  Modulator  curves.  Doli,  rat;  broken  line,  guinea  pig; 

continuous  line,  frog;  O O,  snake.  Equal  quantum  intensity 

spectrum.     Sensitivity     plotted     against    wavelength.      [From 
Granit  (66).] 


given  in  figure  15.  The  modulator  at  5000  A  was 
obtained  after  light-adaptation  from  the  rat,  an  ani- 
mal with  rod  eyes.  In  the  dark-adapted  state  it  had 
been  a  dominator  of  the  ordinary  rhodopsin  type.  It 
is  by  no  means  rare  to  find,  in  practically  pure  rod 
eyes  such  narrow  curves  with  ma.ximum  around  5000 
A,  when  visual  purple  activity  has  been  suppressed 
by  light-adaptation.  The  other  modulators  very 
clearly  occupy  three  regions  of  predilection  which 
have  recurred  since  in  many  other  measurements. 
The  narrowest  modulators  ever  seen  were  found  by 
Donner  (52)  in  the  pigeon  cone  retina  where  they 
also  occurred  in  three  regions  of  predilection  and 
shifted  slightly  towards  wavelengths  which  are  long 
compared  with  those  of  frogs.  Donner  suggested  that 
this  shift  was  due  to  colored  oil  globules.  Modulators 
have  also  been  obtained  by  selective  adaptation  to 
different  wavelengths  as  well  as  by  electrical  polari- 
zation. 

A  much  debated  question  is  whether  these  modu- 
lators represent  the  more  or  less  pure  absorption 
curves  of  photochemical  substances  or  are  products 
of  neural  interaction  based,  for  instance,  on  a  mini- 
mum of  three  broad-band  curves.  The  simplest  basis 
for  such  interaction  would  obviously  be  overlap  of 
liroad-ljand  curves,  such  as  those  of  Dartnall  C35)> 
the  pathways  of  one  set  of  cones  synaptically  sup- 
pressing or  e.xciting  those  of  the  neighbor  cones  with 
which  they  overlap  in  spectral  sensitivity.  As  to  such 
interaction,  it  is  true  that  it  has  been  shown  to  e.xist 
(43,  71),  especiallv  b\'  polarization  methods  (70),  but 


this  does  not  necessarily  constitute  proof  that  it 
actually  did  occur  under  the  circumstances  of  thresh- 
old experiments  of  the  type  used  to  establish  the 
concepts.  On  the  other  hand,  retinal  photochemistry, 
though  highly  developed  in  many  interesting  experi- 
ments by  several  workers  [see  Wald's  summary  in 
Chapter  XXVIII  of  this  volume;  also  Granit  (73, 
75)],  has  not  yet  reached  the  point  when  it  would  be 
possible  to  state  that  narrow-band  photochemical 
substances  do  not  occur  in  living  retinae.  The  most 
that  could  be  said  is  that  broad-band  curves  seem  to 
be  easier  to  demonstrate.  Further  work  will  no  doubt 
solve  this  problem. 

Much  work  has  lately  been  devoted  to  the  study  of 
the  action  of  intermittent  or  flickering  light.  In 
general,  rod  eyes  have  been  found  to  fuse  flicker  at 
lower  values  than  cone  eyes.  In  a  mixed  eye  in  the 
dark  adapted  state  rod  sensitivity  is  high,  for  example 
in  the  frog  retina  with  roughly  equal  numbers  of  rods 
and  cones.  Fusion  frequency  of  the  ERG  to  a  light 
of  some  2000  lux  in  this  animal  will  then  be  around 
7  to  10  flashes  per  sec.  But  if  this  light  is  allowed  to 
shine  for  a  while  so  as  to  light-adapt  the  eye,  the 
fusion  frequency  will  soon  rise  to  values  around  20 
flashes  per  sec.  Now  why  could  not  the  faster  cones 
also  participate  in  tlie  dark-adapted  state  and  raise 
the  fusion  frequency  to  their  higher  rate?  Why  was 
light-adaptation  necessary,  particularly  if  the  ERG 
is  a  pure  receptor  affair  and  not  influenced  by  inter- 
action? Perhaps  the  reply  is  that  interaction  does 
occur  so  that  highly  sensitized  rods  suppress  the 
cones,  as  was  suggested  by  Granit  &  Riddell  (80) 
when  they  made  this  experiment.  They  also  demon- 
strated that  the  flickering  wavelets  change  character 
as  light-adaptation  proceeds  and,  besides,  are  dif- 
ferent in  rod  and  cone  eyes.  The  same  changes  can 
be  seen  in  the  ERG  of  man  (41). 

Figure  16  is  from  experiments  with  ERG  in  guinea 
pigs,  cats  and  pigeons  (45,  49)  and  shows  a  graph  of 
fusion  frequency  against  light  intensity  in  double 
logarithmic  plotting.  Clearly  there  are  two  branches 
of  the  curve  in  cats  and  guinea  pigs.  Much  evidence, 
presented  in  Granit's  summary  (73),  goes  to  show 
that  the  lower  branch  is  a  scotopic  and  the  steeper 
portion  a  photopic  function.  The  less  the  number  of 
cones  (their  being  far  fewer  in  guinea  pigs  than  in 
cat.s),  the  higher  the  intensity  at  the  kink  of  the  curve. 
The  pigeon  with  cone  dominance  in  the  ERG  has 
no  low-intensity  branch  but  the  curve  rises  steeply  to- 
wards values  as  high  as  around  1 30  per  .sec. 

Records  from  the  large  retinal  ganglion  cells  of 
cats  have  shown  (54)  that   fusion   frequenc\'   is  pro- 


NEURAL    ACTIVITY    IN    THE    RETINA 


709 


750r  Fusion  frequency 
iriashesf^^, 

100  ~ 


50 


Light  intensity 


10 


100 


1000 


10000 


FIG.  16.  Double  logarithmic  plot  of  fusion  frequency  of  the 
electroretinogram  against  stimulus  intensity  in  meter  candles. 
O,  cat;  C,  guinea  pig  (two  animals);  •,  pigeon.  [From 
Granit  (73).] 


20' 


SOcylsec 


FIG.  17.  Evcephale  isole  given 
curare.  Collicular  stimulating 
electrodes  on  contralateral  side 
at  depth  H  2  in  Horsley-Clarke 
coordinates.  C,  controls  with  test 
light  of  3  lux  alone.  /,  and  18-20 
show  first  and  three  last  records 
of  stimulation  period  22  sec.  in 
duration,  at  a  rate  of  47  per  sec. 
Note,  no  driving,  as  shown  by 
isolated  shock  artifacts;  after- 
wards diminution  of  spike  fre- 
quency. Sweep  interval  in  sec. 
[From  Granit  (74).] 


portional  to  impulse  frequency  set  up  by  the  indi- 
vidual flashes  just  before  the  moment  of  fusion  when 
impulse  frequency  still  can  be  measured.  Fusion  it- 
self is  defined  as  the  flicker  frequency  at  which  effects 
of  individual  flashes  on  the  spike  frequency  are  no 
longer  discernible.  Flicker  and  fusion  in  electrical 
records  has  recenth'  been  discussed  by  Granit  (73). 


CENTRIFUG.AL    CONTROL 

The  inner  plexiform  layer,  which  forms  a  network 
of  dendrites  between  ganglion  cells  and  bipolar  cells 
densely  interspersed  with  amacrine  cells,  also  re- 
ceives the  terminals  of  the  centrifugal  fibers  (.see  fig. 
2).  Their  central  station  in  the  brain  is  unknown. 
However,  experiments  have  shown  that  it  is  possible 
to  obtain  diff'erent  kinds  of  centrifugal  effects  on  the 
ganglion  cell  discharge  (44,  74).  These  are  partly 
excitatory,  partly  inhibitory  but  quite  often  mixed, 
excitation  followed  by  inhibition,  and  generally  re- 
quire an  array  of  antidromic  stimuli  to  the  optic 
nerve  before  a  definite  effect  is  noticed.  This  is  not 
surprising.  We  are  best  informed  about  centrifugal 
effects  from  the  brainstem  to  the  muscle  spindles  (77) 
through  the  so-called  gamma  neurons  and  these  too 
mostly  require  iterative  stimulation.  Similarly  the 
suppression  of  the  cochlear  nerve  discharge  (58)  by 


stimulation  of  the  centrifugal  olivocochlear  bundle  is 
fully  developed  only  after  it  has  been  stimulated  for 
about  half  a  second  at  the  optimal  rate  of  100  per  sec. 
The  effect  on  the  retina  is  very  .similar  independently 
of  whether  the  site  of  stimulation  is  the  optic  nerve 
where  it  is  spread  out  in  the  pretectum  or  the  brain- 
stem reticular  substance.  In  the  former  case  there  is, 
of  course,  also  driving  of  the  ganglion  cells  by  anti- 
dromic stimulation  which  seems  to  facilitate  excita- 
tory components  on  the  driven  cells.  On  the  other 
hand,  from  lower  portions  of  the  optic  nerve  inhibi- 
tory effects  are  quite  common.  When  the  effect  is 
excitation,  it  tends  to  be  an  increase  of  level  of  excita- 
bility so  that  a  greater  number  of  impulses  are  dis- 
charged between  'on'  and  'off.  The  on-off'  differ- 
entials tend  to  disappear  in  this  general  outburst. 
Again,  when  the  final  result  is  suppression,  the  whole 
effect  of  light  is  suppressed.  An  inhibitory  effect  from 
the  brainstem  reticular  formation  is  shown  in  figure 
17.  High-frequency  on-  or  off-bursts  cannot  be  much 
altered  by  centrifugal  stimulation. 

Recently  further  work  by  Dodt  (44)  on  the  rabbit 
eye  has  led  to  the  actual  demonstration  of  a  centrifu- 
gal spike  picked  up  in  the  retina.  This  spike  is  from 
8  to  20  msec,  delayed  with  respect  to  antidromic  im- 
pulses recorded  from  the  ganglion  cells.  These  also 
are  positive-negative  and  much  larger  than  the  cen- 
trifugal spikes  which  are  purely  negative  as  if  they 
started    below    the    recording    electrode    itself.    It   is 


710 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


impossible  to  elicit  the  ccntriiugal  spike  by  light.  It 
is  at  the  moment  too  early  to  put  forth  a  theory  of 
the  role  of  centrifugal  control. 


ERG    OF    M.^N:    its    CLINICAL    USE 

erg's  of  man  have  been  recorded  from  the  very 
earliest  time  of  electroretinography  but  the  first  really 
good  records  were  published  by  Hartline  (85).  The 
work  gradually  got  under  way,  particularly  when 
the  first  more  differentiated  responses  separating 
rods  and  cones  were  published  by  Motokawa  & 
Mita  (no)  and  Adrian  (i,  2)  and  when  Karpe  (96) 
started  clinical  electroretinography.  In  1956  this 
subject  gathered  a  large  number  of  European  con- 
tinental and  British  students  of  the  human  ERG  to  a 
first  symposium  in  Hamburg  (128).  The  contriljutions 
to  this  symposium  provide  a  convenient  introduction 
to  the  literature  of  electroretinography,  particularly 
in  its  clinical  aspects. 

Since  in  man  it  is  impossible  to  open  the  eye  and 
use  microillumination  of  selected  spots  and,  at  the 
same  dme,  the  dominant  phase  of  the  human  ERG 
is  a  rod  response  requiring  dark-adaptation,  the 
question  of  whether  the  ERG  is  a  generalized  re- 
sponse to  stray  light  or  is  focally  elicited  has  created 
considerable  interest.  Contributions  to  this  discus- 
sion by  Fry  &  Hartley  (57),  Boynton  &  Riggs  (27), 
Asher  (13),  Boynton  (26),  Wirth  &  Zetterstrom 
(150),  Marg  &  Heath  (106)  and  Brindley  (29)  should 


be  consulted.  There  is  general  agreement  that  stray 
light  is  unavoidable  at  the  strength  needed  for  an 
ERG  to  be  recorded  in  the  human  eye,  i.e.  from  the 
cornea;  in  particular,  clinical  work  shows  that  evi- 
dence of  large  scotomata  may  fail  to  appear  in  the 
ERG  (96).  In  cats,  as  stated,  an  area  of  20  mm- 
must  be  illuminated  for  a  maximum  ERG  (150). 
With  regard  to  localized  focal  stimuli  in  opened  eyes 
of  animals  and  whether  they  can  interact  or  not, 
opinions  still  differ  (29,   106). 

Much  recent  theoretical  work  on  the  human  ERG 
has  been  devoted  to  the  identification  of  its  various 
deflections  and  components,  particularly  with  regard 
to  rods  and  cones  (12,  14,  21,  40,  41,  48,  130,  148), 
and  to  a  study  of  its  sensitivity  to  colored  lights  (as 
mentioned  above). 

The  clinical  values  are  both  diagnostic  and  prog- 
nostic. Karpe  (96)  initiated  the  work  iiy  bringing 
together  the  data  necessary  for  establishment  of 
normal  standard  values  for  the  b-wave  and  for  de- 
fining a  number  of  fundamental  pathological  types 
of  initial  deflections.  There  has  since  been  much 
systematic  work  done,  and  in  many  eye  hospitals 
recording  of  the  ERG  is  a  routine  procedure,  not 
only  when  the  eye  media  are  opaque  but  also  for 
the  prognosis  of  varieties  of  tapetoretinal  degenera- 
tions, to  decide  whether  treatment  should  be  surgi- 
cal or  not,  in  children  (151),  etc.  It  seems  that  early 
changes  in  the  ERG  or  failure  of  such  changes  when 
the  ophthalmoscopic  picture  suggests  a  pathological 
retina  are  of  considerable  prognostic  value. 


REFERENCES 


D.   AND  R.   M.ATTHEWS.  J.  Ffiystol.  64:  279. 
D.   AND  R.   Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  65;  273 


1.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.   104:  84,  1945. 

2.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.   105;  24,   1946. 

3.  Adrian,  E.  D.  and  R.  Matthews.  J.  Physiol.  63:  378 
1927. 

4.  Adrian 
1927. 

5.  Adrian 
1928. 

6.  Andree,  G.  and  H.-VV.  Mui.i.er-Limmroth.  J^lschr.  Biol 
106:  395,  1954. 

7.  Arden,  G.  B.  and  D.   p.  Greaves.  J.  Physiol.   133;  266 

8.  Arden,  G.  B.  and  K.  Tanslev.  J.  Physiol.  127:  592,  1955 

9.  Arden,  G.  B.  and  K.  Tanslev.  J.  Physiol.  130:  225,  1955 

10.  Armington,  J.   C.  J.   Opl.  Soc.   Am.  42:   393,    1952. 

11.  Armington,  J.  C.  J.  Opl.  Soc.  Am.  45:  1058,   1955. 

12.  .■\rmington,  J.    C.,   E.    P.    Johnson   and   L.   A.    Riggs 
J.   Physiol.    118:   289,    1952. 

13.  Asher,    H.    J.    Physiol.    112:   40P,    1951. 

14.  AuERBACH,  E.  and  H.  M.  Burian.  Am.  J.  Ophlh.  40:  42 

1955- 

15.  Barlow,  H.  B.  J.  Physiol.    119:  58,   1953. 


16.  Barlow,   H.   B.  J.   Physiol,    iig:  69,    1953. 

17.  Barlow,  H.  B.,  R.  Fitzhugh  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J. 
Physiol.   125;  28P,    1954. 

18.  Benoit,   p.   H.   and   L.   Cornu.   Compt.  raid.  Soc.  de  biol. 

'47:  4,'54.  >953- 
ig.   Bernhard,    C.    G.    and    C.    R.    Skoglund.    Acta  physiol. 

scandinau.  2:   10,    1 941. 

20.  Best,  W.  ^Ischr.  Bwl.   106:   171,   1953. 

21.  Best,   W.   Acta  ophlh.    31;  95,    1953. 

22.  Bishop,  P.  O.,  D.  Jeremy  and  J.  \V.  Lance.  J.  Physiol. 

i-!i:  415.  '953- 

23.  Bishop,  P.  O.  and  J.  S.  O'Le.arv.  J.  .\riirophysiol.  1 :  391, 

'938. 

24.  Boll,   F.   .Moiialsber.  Akad.    W'lss.  Berlin  41:   783,    1876. 

25.  Bornschein,  H.  .Nalurwissenschaften  41  :  435,    1954. 

26.  Boynton,  R.  M.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  43:  442,   1953. 

27.  Boynton,  R.  M.  and  L.  A.  Riggs.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  42: 

217.  >95i- 

28.  Brindley,   G.  S.  J.  Physiol.  134;  339,  1956. 

29.  Brindley,   G.  S.  J.  Physiol.  134:  353,  1956. 

30.  Brindley,   G.  S.  J.  Physiol.  134:   360,  1956. 


NEURAL   ACTIVITY   IN    THE    RETINA 


71 


31.  Chaffee,  E.  L.,  W.  T.  Bovie  and  A.  Hampson.  J.  Opt.  74. 
Soc.  Am.  7;   I,    1923.  75- 

32.  Chaffee,   E.   L.   and  A.   Hampson.  J.   <^pt.  Soc.   .im.   9: 

I,  1924.  76- 

33.  Chaffee,   E.   L.   and   E.   Sutcliffe.   .im    J.   Physiol.   95:  77. 

250.  1930- 

34.  Cobb,  VV.  and  H.  B.  Morton.  EUrlrocmephuhg.  <S  Clin.  78. 
Neurophysiol.  4:  547,  1952. 

35.  Dartnall,  H.  J.  A.  J.  Physiol.  134:  327,  1956.  79. 

36.  Denton,   E.  J.  J.  Physiol.    124:   16P,    1954.  80. 

37.  Denton,   E.  J.  and  J.  H    VVvlhf,.  J.  Physiol.    127:  81,  81. 

1955-  ^^^■ 

38.  Detwiler,    S.     R.     Vertebrate    Photoreceptors.     Ni  w    York: 
Macmillan,  1943.  83. 

39.  DiTCHBURN,  R.  W.  and  B.  L.  Ginsborc.  J.  Physiol,   i  19:  84. 

■■  '953-  ^5- 

40.  DoDT,  E.  von  Graefes  Arch.  Ophlh.    151:  672,   1951.  86. 

41.  DoDT,  E.  von  Graefes  Arch.  Ophth.   153:   152,   1952.  87. 

42.  DoDT,  E.  Expenentia  12:  34,   1956.  88. 

43.  DoDT,  E.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  36:  219,   1956.  89. 

44.  DoDT,  E.  J.  Neurophysiol.    19:  301,    1956.  90. 

45.  DoDT,  E.  AND  C.  Enroth.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  30:  375, 

■953-  91- 

46.  DoDT,  E.  AND  J.  Heck.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  259:  212,  1954. 

47.  DoDT,  E.  AND  J.  Heck.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  259:  226,  1954. 

48.  DoDT,  E.  AND  L.  Wadensten.  Acta  ophth.  32:  165,   1954.  92. 

49.  DoDT,   E.   and  a.   VVirth.   Acta  physiol.  scandinav.   30:  80, 

1953-  93- 

50.  DoGIEL,   A.   S.   iirch.  mikroskop.   Anal.   44:  622,    1895. 

51.  DoNNER,    K.   O.    Acta  physiol.    scandinav.    21:    Suppl.    72,  94. 

19.50.  95- 

52.  DoNNER,  K.  O.  J.  Physiol.   122:  524,   1953.  96. 
33.   EiNTHOVEN,    W.    and    W.    A.   JoLLY.    Quart.    J.    Exper.  97. 

Physiol.  I  :  373,  1 908.  98. 

54.  Enroth,    Ch.    Acta    physiol.    scandinav.    27:    Suppl.     100, 

195^-  99- 

55.  Forbes,   A.,   S.   Burleigh   and   M.   Nevland.  J.   jVeuro- 
physiol.  18:  517,  1955.  100. 

56.  Frolioh,  F.  W.  Grundziige  einer  Lehre  vom  Lichl-  mid  loi. 
Farhensinn.  Ein  Beitrag  zur  allgemeinen  Physiologic  der  Sinne.  102. 
Jena:  Fischer,    1921,  86  pp. 

57.  Fry,  G.  a.  and  S.  H.  B.^rtley.  Am.  J.  Plnstnl.  111:  335,  103. 

1935- 

58.  Galambos,  R.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   19:  424,   1956.  104. 

59.  Gebhard,   J.    W.    Proc.    29th    Meeting    Armed    Forces 
National   Research   Council  Vision   Committee,    1951.  105. 

60.  Gotch,  F.  J.  Physiol.  29:  388,  1903. 

61.  Gotch,  F.  J.  Physiol.  31-  i,   1904.  105a 

62.  Goto,  M.  and  N.  Toida.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  4:  123,  1954. 

63.  Goto,  M.  and  N.  Toida.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  4:  221,   1954.  106. 

64.  Granit,  R.  J.  Physiol.  77:  207,    1933.  107. 

65.  Granit,  R.  J.  Physiol.  85:  421,    1935.  108. 
65a. Granit,  R.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  2:  334,  1941.  log. 

66.  Granit,   R.  Nature,  London    151;   11,    1943. 

67.  Granit,  R.  J.  Physiol.  103:  103,  1944.  110. 

68.  Granit,  R.  Proc.  Phys.  Soc.  London  57:  447,    1945. 

69.  Granit,  R.  Sensory  Mechanisms  of  the  Retina.  London:  iii. 
Oxford,   1947,  412  pp. 

70.  Granit,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.   11:  253,   1948.  112. 

71.  Granit,  R.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.   18;  281,    1949. 

72.  Granit,  R.  Annee  P'.ychol.  50:  129,   1951.  113. 

73.  Granit,   R.   Receptors  and  Sensory  Perception.   New  Haven: 

Yale,  1955,  367  pp.  114. 


Granit,  R.  J.  .Neurophysiol.    18:  388,   1955. 

Granit,    R.    Proc.    XXth    Internat.    Physiol.    Congress, 

Brussels,  1956,  p.  65. 

Granit,  R.  and  T.  Helme.  J.  Neurophysiol.  a:  556,  1939. 

Granit,  R.  .^nd  B.  R.  Kaad.\.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  27: 

130,  1952. 

Granit,  R.  and  A.  Munsterhjelm.  J.  Physiol.  88:  436, 

1937- 

Granit,  R.  and  C.  G.  Phillips.  J.  Physiol.  133;  520,  1956. 
Granit,  R.  and  L.  A.  Riddell.  J.  Physiol.  81:  i,  1934. 
Granit,  R.  and  P.  O.  Therman.  J.  Physiol.  83:  359,  1935. 
Granit,    R.    and   P.    O.   Therman.   J.    Physiol.  91:    127, 

'937- 

Granit,  R.  and  P.  O.  Therman.  J.  Physiol.  93:  gP,  1938. 

Granit,  R.  and  C.  M,  Wrede.  J.  Physiol.  89:  239,  1937. 

Hartline,   H.   K.   Am.  J.  Physiol.   73:  600,    1925. 

Hartline,  H.   K.  Am.  J.  Physiol.    121:  400,    1938. 

Hartline,  H.  K.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   130:  6go,   1940. 

Hartline,  H.  K.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.   130:  700,   1940. 

Hartline,  H.  K.  J.  Opt.  Soc.  Am.  30:  239,   1940. 

Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  F.  Ratliff.  J. 

Gen.   Physiol.   39:  651,    1956. 

Hartline,  H.  K.,  H.  G.  Wagner  and  T.  Tomita.  Proc. 

XlXth     Internat.     Physiol.     Congress,    Montreal,     1953, 

p.  441. 

Hecht,   S.,    S.    Shlaer    and   M.    H.    Pirenne.   J.    Gen. 

Phyuol.  25:  819,  1942. 

Heck,  J.  and  I.  Rend.'Mil.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  39:  167, 

1957- 

Holmgren,  F.   Vpsala  Idkaref.  frirh.   i:   177,   1865-66. 

Holmgren,  F.  Upsala  Idkaref.  fdrh.  6:  419,   1870-71. 

Karpe,  G.  Acta  Ophth.  Suppl.  24:   ig45,  118  pp. 

Katz,  B.  J.  Physiol,  in:  261,   igso. 

Kohlrausch,    a.    Hnndh.    nnim.   path.    Physiol.    12,    Pt.    2: 

1393.  193'- 

Konig,     a.     Gesammelte    Abhandlungen     zur    physiologischen 

Optik.  Leipzig;  Barth,   1903,  443  pp. 

Kuffler,  S.  W.  J.  Neurophysiol.    16:  37,    1953. 

KiJHNE,  W.  Handb.  Physiol.  3:  235,  1879. 

KiJHNE,  W.   AND  J.  Steiner.  Unlcrs.  physiol.  Inst.  Heidelb. 

3:  327,  1880. 

KiJHNE,  VV.  AND  J.  Steiner.   Unters.  physiol.  Inst.  Heidelb. 

4:  64,  1881. 

Lorente   de   No,   R.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.   Physiol.   29:  207, 

■947- 

MacNichol,   E.   F. 
1956. 

.MacNichol,   E.  J 
Ophlh.  46:  26,  1958. 

Marg,  E.  and  G.  G.  Heath.  Science  122:   1234,   1955. 
Marshall,  W.  H.  J.  Neurophysiol.   12:  277,   1949. 
Motokawa,  K.  J.  Neurophysiol.    12:  291,   ig4g. 
Motokawa,   K.,   K.   Iwama  and  S.  Tukahara.    Tohoku 
J.  Exper.  Med.  53:  3gg,  igsi. 

MoTOK.wVA,  K.  and  T.  Mita.  Tohoku  J.  Exper.  Med.  42: 
1 14,  1942. 

Muller-Limmroth,  H.-W.  and  G.  Andree.  ^tschr.  Biol. 
105:  348,  1952. 

MiJLLER-LiMMROTH,  H.-W.  AND  G.  Andree.  Arch.  ges. 
Physiol.  257:  216,   1953. 

Muller-Limmroth,  H.-W.  and  W.  Wirth.  ^tschr.  Biol. 
107.  444,  1955. 
NoELL,  W.  K.  Studies  on  the  Electrophysiology  and  the  Me- 


AND  R.   Benolken.  Science  124:  681, 
Jr.    and    G.    Svaetichin.    Am.    J. 


712 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


lahn/isni    of    the    Reliiia.     Randolph    Field    Texas;    USAF  133. 

School  of  Aviation  Medicine,   1953,   122  pp.  134. 

115.  Ottoson,    D.    and    G.    SvAETicHiN.    Cold    Sprinii    Harbor  135. 
Symp.  Qiianl.  Biol.  17:  165,  195J.  136. 

116.  Ottoson,  D.  and  G.  Svaetichin.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav. 
Suppl.    106,  29:  538,   1954.  137- 

117.  Parinaud,  H.  La   Vision.  Paris:  Doin,    1898,  218  pp. 

118.  Parry,   H.    B.,    K.   Tanslev   and   L.    C.   Thomson.   J.  138. 
Physiol.  120:28,  1953. 

119.  Piper,  H.  Arch.  Anal.  Physiol.  .Suppl.  Bd.   1905,  p.   133.  139. 

120.  Piper,  H.   Arch.  Anal.   Physiol.  Suppl.   Bd.    1910,   p.   461. 

121.  Piper,    H.    Arch.    Anal.    Physiol.    1911,    p.    85.  140. 

122.  PoLVAK,   S.    The  Retina.    Chicago:   Uni\'.    Chicago   Press.  141. 
1 94 1,  607  pp.  '4' 3 

123.  Ram6n  y  Cajal,  S.  Die  Relinn  der  Wirbelliere.  Wiesbaden:  142. 
Bergmann,  1894,  168  pp. 

124.  Ramon  y  Cajal,  S.    Trai'.  Lah.  Rech.   Biol.   Untv.  .Madrid  143. 
28:  Appendice,  1933.  I44- 

125.  RiOGS,  L.  A.,  F.  Ratliff,  j.  C.  Corn.sweet  and  T.  N. 
Cornsweet.  J.   Opt.  Sac.  Am.  43:  495,    1953.  I45- 

126.  Ronchi,  L.  and  S.  Gr.\zi.   Istituto  Nazionale  di  Ottica, 
Firenze,  Technical  Note  No.   5,    1956.  I4*'- 

127.  RusHTON,  W.  A.  H.  J.  Physiol.   134:  30,   1956. 

128.  Sautter,  H.  and  W.  Straub  (editors).  Elektroretinographie.  147. 
Basel:  Karger,  1957.  14*^- 

129.  Schmidt,  W.  J.  Kolloid-Zlidir.  85:  137,  1938.  149. 

130.  Schubert,  G.   .\nd  H.   Bornschein.   Ophthalmologica   123:  150. 

396.  1952- 

131.  ScHULTZE,    M.    .Sinckers   Handh.   d.    Lehre   von   den   Geweben.  151. 
2:977,  1871. 

132.  Sjostrand,  F.  S.  7.  Appl.  Physiol.   19:   1188,   1948. 


Sjostrand,  F.  S.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  33:  383,    1949. 

Sjostrand,  F.  S.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  42:   15,   1953. 

Sjostrand,  F.  S.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  42:  45,   1953. 

Stiles,    VV.    S.    and   B.    H.    Crawford.    Proc.    Roy.    .Soc, 

London,  ser.  B  112:  428,   1933. 

Svaetichin,    G.    Acta  physiol.    scandinav.    Suppl.    106,    29: 

601,  1954. 

Tansley,   K.,   and   B.   K.  Johnson.   .Vatiire,   London    178: 

1285,  1956. 

Therman,   p.  O.  Ada  Soc.  Scient.  Fenn.  Nova  Ser.    B.  2 : 

I,  1938. 

Toida,  N.   and  M.  Goto.  Jap.  J.   Physiol.  4;  260,    1954. 
ToMiTA,   T.  Jap.   J.   Physiol,    i:    110,    1950. 
.Tomita,  T.  Jap.  J.  Physiol.  7:  80,  1957. 
ToMiTA,  T.   AND  Y.   Torihama.  Jap.  J.   Physiol.  6:    118, 

1956. 

Tukah.\ra,  S.    Tohoku  J.  Exper.  Med.  54:   11,    1951- 

VON  Brucke,  E.  T.  and  S.  Garten.  .\rch.  ges.  Physiol.  120: 

290,  1907. 

von  Kries,  J.    Handb.  norm.  path.    Physiol.    12.  Pt.    1:678, 

1929. 

Wal.ls,  G.  L.   The  \'ertebrate  Eye  and  its  Adaptive  Radiation. 

Michigan:  Cranbrook  Press,    1942,   785  pp. 

VViLLMER,  E.  N.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.   17:  339,   1955. 

WiRTH,  A.  von  Graefes  Arch.  Ophlh.  151:  662,  1951. 

Wirth,  a.  Arch.  sc.  biol.  40:   163,    1956. 

WiRTH,  .\.  AND  B.  Zetterstrom.  Brit.  J.  Ophlh.  38:  257, 

'954- 

Zetterstrom,  B.  .Studies  on  the  Postnatal  Development  of  the 
Eleclroretinooram  in  .Newborn  Infants.  .Stockholm :  Akademisk 
.-\\  handling  Karolinska  Institutet,  1956. 


CHAPTER    XXX 


Central  mechanisms  of  vision 


S.    HOWARD   BARTLEY     |     Department  of  Psychology,  Michigan  State  University,  East  Lansing,  Michigan 


CHAPTER     G  O  N  T  E  N  I  S 

Types  of  Data 

Commonality  in  Modes  of  Study 
Phenomena  of  Vision 
Phenomena  of  the  Optic  Pathway 

Brain  waves;  spontaneous  and  evoked 

Optic  pathway 

Cortical  response 

Activation  of  cortex  by  stimulation  of  radiation 

Activity  in  regions  other  than  optic  cortex 

Cortical  localization 

Properties  of  dendrites 
Visual  Phenomena  to  be  Explained 

Gross  Response  to  Gross  Intensity  Relations 

Area  of  Target 

Brightness 

Flicker  and  Fusion 

Brightness  Enhancement 

Bilateral  Functions 

Brightness  Contrast 

Visual  Movement 

Color  Vision 


THE  PRESENT  CHAPTER,  devotcd  to  Central  mechanisms 
of  vision,  must  deal  with  two  diverse  sets  of  phe- 
nomena. One  set  comprises  the  phenomena  that, 
taken  together,  we  call  vision.  The  other  is  the  group 
of  neurophysiological  phenomena  that  constitute  the 
activities  of  the  central  end  of  the  optic  pathway.  The 
visual  phenomena  must  be  considered  first,  for  they 
are  the  items  to  be  accounted  for,  if  possible,  by  what 
we  know  about  the  optic  pathway  and  its  associated 
systems. 

Vision  is  the  behavior  of  the  organism  that  stems 
more  or  less  directly  from  optic  pathway  activity. 
Vision  includes  both  the  introspective  (the  experien- 
tial) and  the  motor.  Visual  behavior  in  question  is 


divisible  in  still  another  way.  Part  of  it  is  the  immedi- 
ate discriminatory  reaction  which  we  call  visual  per- 
ception. Perception  is  not  only  experiential  but  also 
motor  in  expression.  Another  part  of  behavior  is  in 
the  form  of  imagery,  etc.,  that  is,  a  function  of  the 
visual  mechanism  when  the  eye  is  not  stimulated. 

Material  appropriate  in  the  discussion  of  central 
visual  mechanisms  stems  first  from  what  we  know 
about  visual  phenomena  in  accord  with  the  definition 
of  vision  just  given  and  from  what  we  have  found  out 
from  direct  investigation  of  the  activity  of  the  entire 
optic  pathway.  We  may  also  legitimately  include  cer- 
tain inferences  that  seem  to  be  necessary  to  bridge  the 
gaps  in  our  knowledge  and  to  provide  a  basis  for 
further  investigation. 


TYPES    OF    D.«iTA 

In  this  portion  of  the  chapter  we  must  specify,  at 
least  in  general,  the  kinds  of  phenomena  with  which 
we  shall  deal.  They  are  of  two  kinds  so  diverse  that 
they  generally  are  dealt  with  in  entirely  separate  dis- 
courses. The  one  class  is  visual  and  includes  the  ex- 
periential outcomes  of  the  action  of  the  optic  pathway 
activated  by  photic  radiation.  The  other  class  of 
phenomena  is  neurophysiological.  The  task  of  the 
present  chapter  seeins  to  be  one  of  relating  the  two 
classes  of  phenomena. 


Commonality  in  Modes  of  Study 

The  fundamental  requirement  in  relating  vision  to 
the  various  phenomena  in  the  optic  pathway  is  for 
the  two  sets  of  events  to  be  initiated  by  the  same  ex- 
ternal event  (stimulus).  In  this  way,  one  can  .say  that 


7'3 


714 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


a  given  visual  end  result  is  occasioned  upon  such  and 
such  events  in  the  bodily  mechanism  involved. 

It  is  fortunate  that  the  same  stimulus  conditions 
can  and  have  been  used  to  study  the  behavior  of  the 
two  categories  of  events.  This  is  to  say  that  not  only 
are  photic  stimuli  used  in  both  cases,  but  the  very 
same  manipulations  are  used  and  seem  effective  in 
giving  us  the  data  we  need. 

The  following  remarks  have  to  do  with  modes  of 
studying  both  vision  and  the  neural  mechanisms  that 
underlie  them.  Vision  can  be  studied  only  in  the  intact 
or  near-intact  animal.  Neurophysiological  mecha- 
nisms can  be  studied  in  the  reduced  animal  and  in 
animal  preparations.  Vision  is  to  be  studied  only  by 
use  of  retinal  stimulation  with  photic  radiation. 
Neurophysiological  mechanisms  (including  central 
mechanisms)  may  be  studied  by  stimulating  either 
the  retina  or  by  eliminating  it  and  stimulating  the 
optic  nerve  directly  by  electrical  energy,  or  by  directly 
stimulating  regions  farther  along  in  the  pathway.  The 
electrical  method  provides  for  stricter  control  than 
the  photic  and,  though  'unnatural'  in  the  temporal 
pattern  of  impulses  delivered,  the  central  end  of  the 
pathway  is  helpful  in  analyzing  the  nature  of  the 
mechanisms  involved.  Eliminating  the  retina  elimi- 
nates the  selective  feature^  employed  by  it  in  pro- 
ducing the  optic  nerve  discharge.  The  retina  selects 
or  emphasizes  certain  channels  in  the  optic  nerve 
according  to  the  distribution  of  the  discharge  into  the 
various  channels  in  keeping  with  its  own  principles  of 
divergence  and  convergence  in  its  neural  circuits. 
Virtually  no  discharge  initiated  by  the  retina  involves 
all  channels  simultaneously.  They  are  activated  in 
temporal  succession  of  some  sort  or  another. 

When  the  optic  nerve  is  stimulated  directly 
(electrically),  all  available  channels  may  be  activated 
together  in  time  and  thus  a  very  different  reception 
of  the  afferent  input  may  occur  at  the  stations  along 
the  pathway.  To  discover  just  how  nearly  alike  the 
two  forms  of  stimulation,  in  effect,  can  be  is  a  matter 
of  empirical  test. 

At  this  point,  it  may  be  appropriate  to  point  out 
one  of  the  more  salient  features  of  the  retina-initiated 
optic  nerve  discharge,  namely  that  it  is  composed  of 
three  temporal  orders,  a)  One  is  the  maintained  dis- 
charge, a  series  of  impulses  lasting  throughout  the  life 
of  the  photic  impingement  on  the  retina.  6)  Another 
is  the  on-off  discharge,  occurring  at  the  beginning  and 
also  at  the  termination  of  the  photic  impingement, 
c)  The  third  type  is  the  off  discharge,  occurring  only 
following  the  termination  of  impingement.  There  are 
certain  visual  end  results  that  seem  to  be  explained 


on  the  basis  of  these  differences.  The  retina  is  also 
responsible  for  an  unexpected  end  result,  the  seeing 
of  two  flashes  when  the  photic  impingement  (pulse) 
is  brief,  moderate  in  intensity  and  singular,  owing  to 
the  fact  that  the  two  sets  of  sense  cells  do  not  have 
the  same  latency. 

Modes  of  study  in\olve  not  only  the  two  forms  of 
setting  up  the  optic  nerve  message  but  also  manipula- 
tions in  the  photic  impingements  themselves.  In 
general,  three  forms  of  timing  may  be  employed :  a) 
single  i-solated  stimuli;  /;)  paired  stimuli,  in  which  the 
two  members  of  the  pair  are  variously  separated  in 
time;  and  r)  trains  of  stimuli,  often  called  intermittent 
stimulation.  In  intermittent  stimulation,  time  inter- 
vals between  stimuli  may  be  varied,  and  the  ratio 
between  the  stimulus  (pulse)  duration  and  the  length 
of  the  cycle  of  intermittency  may  also  be  varied. 

These  three  forms  of  manipulation  have  turned  out 
to  be  much  more  than  empty  differences  in  form  of 
stimulation  as  will  be  seen  later.  The  use  of  method 
a  provides  for  a  response  from  a  resting  system,  at 
least  as  far  as  intended  activation  is  concerned. 
Method  b  proxides  for  the  determination  of  the  effect 
of  the  first  stimulus  on  the  second,  or  otherwise  stated 
it  provides  for  discovering  how  long  it  takes  for  the 
reacting  system  to  complete  its  response  and  reassume 
status  quo.  Method  c  provides  for  still  another  aspect 
of  the  reacting  system  to  become  manifest.  Since  the 
optic  pathway  consists  of  a  number  of  parallel  chan- 
nels, each  with  finite  limits  in  the  rate  at  which  it  can 
be  reactivated,  it  is  possible  that,  when  a  whole  train 
of  stimuli  is  dcli\ered  at  a  rate  beyond  which  single 
channels  can  repeatedly  respond,  a  redistribution  of 
the  relationships  between  repeated  pulses  and  the 
responses  to  them  occurs  as  stimulation  progresses. 

Phenomena  of  Vision 

The  phenomena  of  vision  are  the  items  to  be  ulti- 
mately accounted  for,  hence  it  is  necessary  that  we 
have  in  mind  what  they  are.  Vision  consists  in  the 
appreciation  of  the  surrounds  via  the  use  of  the  eye, 
the  nervous  system,  and  in  turn  the  effector  muscles. 
The  feature  of  the  environment  to  which  response  is 
made  is,  of  course,  photic  radiation.  The  dimensions 
involved  are  spatial,  intensive  and  temporal.  Hence 
it  could  easily  be  supposed  that  these  w-oujd  be  the 
experimental  variables  to  be  used. 

In  vision  a  field  is  responded  to  in  terms  of  intensive 
components  that,  when  they  evoke  experience,  are 
perceived  as  lightness  and  darkness  of  various  degrees. 
These  qualities  need  not  be  stable  but  may  be  per- 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


715 


ceived  as  appearing  and  disappearing.  \'ision  is  also 
response  to  spatial  rclationsiiips  of  radiation  origi- 
nating in  various  directions  from  the  eye.  When  these 
relationships  are  experienced,  we  see  objects  at  various 
locations,  manifesting  various  inovements  and  assum- 
ing various  directions  from  us.  We  are  also  visually 
responsive  to  manipulations  in  timing  of  radiation 
coming  from  various  parts  of  the  field.  When  this 
response  is  in  the  form  of  experience,  events  are  seen 
as  occurring  in  succession,  or  together  in  time,  and 
lasting  for  various  durations.  Photic  radiation  may 
also  be  differentially  responded  to  in  terms  of  its 
wavelength.  The  experience  then  is  of  color,  hue, 
saturation  and  brightness. 

It  is  meant  to  be  clear  to  the  reader  that  vision, 
i.  e.  perceptual  response,  may  occur  in  the  form  of 
clear  immediate  experience,  in  the  form  of  inarticulate 
gross  incipient  reactions  or  in  the  form  of  articulate 
differential  motor  responses.  In  the  last  analysis,  the 
sharp  dichotomizing  between  the  experiential  and  the 
motor  is  only  one  of  the  possible  ways  of  dealing  with 
beha\ior.  It  would  seem  that  when  one  looks  carefully 
at  the  kinds  of  behavior  which  the  human  manifests, 
it  is  more  appropriate  to  postulate  a  kind  of  spectrum 
of  kinds  rather  than  two  opposite  kinds  with  no  form 
intervening  between  them. 

The  following  are  some  of  the  essential  types  of 
problems  which  we  must  handle  as  best  we  can  with 
our  present  information  and  interpretations,  a)  The 
first  problem  is  that  of  seeing  various  brightnesses, 
i)  Next  we  have  the  problem  of  seeing  continuously. 
It  is  known  that  many  neurophysiological  processes 
are  discontinuous.  Were  these  the  only  processes  we 
could  discover  in  the  nervous  system,  the  problem  of 
getting  continuity  from  discontinuity  would  seem  to 
be  a  cardinal  one.  We  are  beginning  to  learn  of  sus- 
tained activity  or  sustained  state  of  potential,  and 
this  may  help  a  great  deal,  c)  This  is  followed  by  the 
problem  of  differential  response  to  various  parts  of  the 
space  field,  since  response  to  the  field  as  a  whole  is 
not  what  would  be  expected  were  the  response  to  iso- 
lated parts  either  independent  or  summative.  d)  The 
problem  of  fine  resolution  is  the  question  of  how 
closely  adjacent  parts  of  the  field  are  seen  as  separate. 
i)  Finally,  we  have  the  problem  of  differential  re- 
sponse to  various  parts  of  the  spectrum — both  local 
response  to  an  isolated  part  of  the  field  and  response 
to  the  whole  field. 

While  the  problems  that  ha\e  just  been  listed  are 
fundamental,  they  are  in  the  form  of  generalizations 
and  cannot  be  dealt  with  as  directly  and  as  concretely 
as  is  the  case  when  particular  visual  phenomena  are 


chosen.  For  most  of  our  considerations,  we  have  chosen 
phenomena  that  are  fairly  specific  but  stand  for  the 
broader  classes  to  which  they  belong. 

The  first  general  phenomenon  appropriate  for  men- 
tion is  that  of  gross  response  to  the  simplest  major 
intensity  differentiations  in  the  field.  This  is  the 
response  merely  to  one  large  part  of  the  field  as  more 
intense  than  the  others. 

W'hereas  the  foregoing  item  may  be  thought  to 
have  to  do  with  brightness,  brightness  is  an  experi- 
ence. It  and  any  overt  response  that  seems  to  be 
related  to  it  in  an  experimentally  approachable  way 
had  better  be  put  in  a  class  by  themselves.  Hence,  in 
the  present  category,  we  refer  to  the  higher  order  re- 
sponses to  intensity  relations  in  the  stimulus  field. 
These  responses  would  be  expected  to  be  based  on 
cortical  function,  whereas  those  in  category  above  may 
be  subcortical. 

The  observer  experiences  undulations  of  darkness 
and  lightness  in  temporal  sequence.  This  is  flicker.  At 
high  rates  of  intermittent  stimulation  this  experience 
is  lost,  a  fact  implying  that  the  neural  or  some  other 
mechanism  is  unable  to  keep  pace.  Differential  re- 
sponse to  the  intermittent  stimulation  of  the  optic 
mechanisms  of  subhuman  species  also  are  quite 
common  and  manifest  many  close  parallels  or  simi- 
larities to  the  quantitative  features  of  the  experiential 
responses  of  the  human  subject. 

Brightness  enhancement  is  another  feature  of 
human  experiential  response.  It  is  the  case  in  which 
intermittent  stimulation  results  in  a  higher  brightness 
than  continuous  stimulation  of  the  same  intensity. 

Bilateral  functions  involving  the  use  of  the  two  eyes 
result,  of  course,  in  a  different  input  into  the  central 
nervous  system  than  the  involvement  of  one  eye  alone. 
It  has  been  found  that  both  the  experiential  and  the 
oculomotor  outcomes  differ  in  the  two  cases. 

Brightness  contrast  occurs  when  fields  made  up  of 
certain  patterns  and  intensities  of  radiation  are  pre- 
sented. They  are  reacted  to  in  ways  not  predictable 
from  the  separate  local  intensities  of  the  parts  of  the 
field.  These  parts  are  not  independent  in  effect  nor 
are  the  results  simply  additive  when  they  are  interde- 
pendent. The  major  phenomena  in  this  category  are 
often  known  as  brightness-contrast  phenomena. 

Visual  movement  creates  complexities.  Portions  of 
the  visual  field  are  not  stable.  They  quickly  appear 
and  disappear  as  segregated  portions  that  may  or  may 
not  undergo  spatial  displacement.  When  movement 
is  seen  under  conditions  where  no  visual  target  ele- 
ments are  displaced,  it  has  been  customary  to  call  the 
movement    'apparent    movement.'    When    displace- 


7i6 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


ment  is  involved,  the  movement  is  said  to  be  'real.' 
Again  it  may  be  said  that  subhuman  species  respond 
in  motor  ways  so  as  to  indicate  they  are  differentially 
sensitiv-e  both  in  cases  of  displacement  and  in  cases  of 
stationary  localization  of  targets. 

Color  vision  is  the  name  given  to  the  fact  that  both 
human  and  some  subhuman  species  give  evidence  of 
being  differentially  sensitive  to  various  portions  of  the 
visible  spectrum.  Color  as  an  experience  may  also  be 
exoked  in  the  human  by  nonspectrally  selected  radia- 
tion, hence  the  color  end  result  stems  also  from  condi- 
tions of  nonspectral  selection. 

Phenomena  of  the  Optic  Palliivnx 

BRAIN  waves:  spontaneous  and  EVOKED.  One  of  the 
major  considerations  in  dealing  with  the  neurophysi- 
ology of  central  phenomena,  'brain  waves,'  for  exam- 
ple, is  the  assignment  of  their  origin.  All  the  activities 
which  we  deal  with  can  be  put  into  two  classes :  those 
that  occur  when  no  intended  peripheral  input  to  the 
brain  is  involved  (spontaneous  activity),  and  the  spe- 
cific activity  that  occurs  when  known  inputs  are 
delivered  through  intended  stimulation  (specific  or 
evoked  responses). 

What  do  the  characteristic  waves  found  in  the 
record  of  spontaneous  activity  represent?  What  ele- 
ments produce  them?  There  are  two  quite  obvious 
alternative  possibilities.  One  is  that  these  rhythmic 
patterns  of  potential  are  the  summed  record  of  the 
primary  impulses  of  unit  neuronal  responses  occurring 
somewhat  out  of  phase  and  producing  wave  envelopes 
of  much  longer  duration  than  the  unit  impulses  them- 
selves. The  second  alternati\e  is  that  the  recorded 
waves  are  the  manifestations  of  slower  longer-lasting 
processes  that  are  more  nearly  similar  in  duration  to 
the  recorded  waves  than  would  be  the  case  in  the  first 
alternative.  These  waves  would  apparently  be  some- 
thing like  after-potentials  in  elements  where  activity 
would  fall  short  of  the  kind  of  discharge  that  produces 
spikes.  Bishop  &  Clare  (21)  believe  that  the  first  al- 
ternative is  preferable  in  accounting  primarily  for 
spontaneous  activity.  They  believe  that  this  activity 
may  incidentally  include  slower  potentials  suggested 
in  the  second  alternati\  e.  As  for  e\oked  responses,  the 
slow  surface-negative  portions  may  be  an  example  of 
a  slow  potential  of  the  character  suggested  in  the 
second  alternative. 

Whichever  alternative  may  operate,  the  next  ques- 
tion is  whether  the  spontaneous  and  the  evoked  activi- 
ties as  the  result  of  well-controlled  peripheral  stimula- 


tion occupy  the  same  cortical  elements.  Of  course, 
there  are  two  alternatives  here.  At  first,  it  was  inferred 
that  they  did  not,  but  it  has  later  appeared  possi- 
ble that  the  two  activities  share  at  least  some  common 
elements  (15,  21). 

The  conclusions  just  given  are  in  line  with  the  find- 
ings of  Adrian  &  Moruzzi  (i).  They  reported  that 
groups  of  impulses  were  discharged  via  axons  in  the 
pyramidal  tract  in  unison  with  the  alpha  cycle  of  the 
motor  cortex.  Thus  it  appears  that  whatev-er  may  be 
said  about  the  cortical  waves  them.selves,  impulse 
volleys  are  associated  with  them.  Primary  impulses  of 
cortical  cells  are  involved  in  spontaneous  activity. 
The  conclusions  are  also  consistent  with  the  finding 
of  Bartley  (4)  that,  following  the  cortical  response  to 
the  afferent  input  via  the  optic  nerve  and  radiation, 
the  cortex  is  refractory  to  a  second  stimulus,  the  de- 
gree depending  upon  elapsed  time.  The  moment  of 
full  recovery  coincides  with  the  point  at  which  the 
alpha-like  portion  of  the  typical  evoked  response 
de\elops.  This  alpha-like  portion  may  be  spoken  of  as 
a  sequel  to  the  specific  response  (21)  or  be  considered 
as  a  less  specific  but  true  portion  of  the  response.  The 
conclusions  are  also  in  line  with  Bishop's  (18)  finding 
that  responses  to  stimulation  of  the  optic  nerve  waxed 
and  waned  in  such  a  way  as  to  imply  that  the  sponta- 
neous alpha  wave  left  a  depression  of  the  same  tem- 
poral character  as  the  evoked  response  just  mentioned 

(4). 

Interpretations  that  may  be  added  in  this  connec- 
tion are  those  of  Bremer  (31),  Eccles  (40),  and  Gastaut 
el  al.  (42).  The  first  of  these  attributed  spontaneously 
occurring  rhythmic  potentials  in  connection  with 
excitability  changes,  plus  axon  discharges  to  account 
for  the  brain  waves  observed  in  'resting'  records.  The 
second  supposes  that  the  activity  in  the  cortex  can  be 
interpreted  as  being  analogous  to  that  in  the  spinal 
cord  where  neuronal  activity  involves  reco\ery  from 
depression.  The  interpretation  is  that  rhythmicity 
results  from  successive  re-excitations  following  periods 
of  depression.  Apparently  closed  neural  chains  form 
the  sources  of  the  re-excitations.  The  third,  in  study- 
ing what  is  ordinarih-  called  photic  driving  of  the 
cortex,  inferred  that  the  spontaneous  cycle  is  an  ex- 
pression of  refractoriness  following  discharge. 

OPTIC  pathway.  The  optic  pathway  consists  of:  a) 
the  tract,  including  the  optic  nerve;  />)  the  relay  nuclei 
of  the  lateral  geniculate,  the  pretectal  area  and  the 
superior  colliculus;  c)  the  radiation  to  cortex,  and 
paths  to  thalamus  and  tectal  area;  and  d)  the  projec- 
tion areas  including  the  cortical,  thalamic  and  tectal 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


717 


projections.  In  addition  to  these,  there  are  e)  the 
association  areas.  Just  where  to  delimit  the  visual  sys- 
tem is  problematic,  depending  upon  how  one  views 
neural  functions. 

Omitting  retinal  structures,  the  first  way  station  is 
the  geniculate  body.  Saggitally,  the  dorsal  nucleus  of 
the  lateral  geniculate  body  of  the  cat  possesses  three 
layers :  A,  Aj  and  B.  The  middle  layer  has  been  further 
diflferentiated  by  Rioch  (62).  Layers  A  and  B  receive 
terminals  of  tract  fibers  of  the  contralateral  retina. 
The  middle  layer,  Aj,  is  the  terminus  of  fibers  from 
the  homolateral  retina.  It  would  seem  that  the  de- 
velopment of  binocular  vision  has  involved  increased 
stratification  of  the  dorsal  nucleus  of  the  geniculate. 
The  rabbit  nucleus  possesses  scarcely  any,  if  any,  and 
those  of  monkey  and  man  present  six  layers.  Part  of 
the  process  of  development  seems  to  have  involved  an 
increase  in  homolateral  representation  of  the  retina. 

There  are  four  groups  of  fibers  in  the  optic  tract 
(24).  These  groups  distribute  to  four  different  regions 
and  are  unlike  in  range  of  cross-section  size  and  in 
conduction  rate.  The  fastest  conducting  groups  inner- 
vate layers  A  and  A]  of  the  lateral  geniculate.  These 
fibers  relay  to  the  projection  area  of  the  striate  cortex. 


CORTEX] 


PRETECTAL. 


THALAMUS 


GENICULATE 


CULUS 


FIG.  I.  Diagram  to  indicate  distribution  of  optic  activity  to 
structures  beyond  the  optic  tract.  The  first  relay  neurons  in  the 
geniculate,  pretectal  area  and  colliculus  are  indicated  for  four 
tract  components.  Neurons  for  projection  are  represented  in 
the  striate  cortex,  lateral  nucleus  of  thalamus  and  tectum. 
From  the  striate  cortex  paths  are  indicated  to  elaborative 
structures  of  opposite  cortex,  association  cortex,  pulvinar,  etc. 
In  cortex  a  short-axon  cell  is  inferred  on  the  basis  of  other 
work  to  relay  the  impulse  from  afferent  fiber  to  pyramid  cell. 
[From  Bishop  &  Clare  (24).] 


The  next  slower  group  synapses  in  layer  B  of  the 
geniculate,  and  relays  to  the  lateral  nucleus  of  the 
thalamus.  The  third  group  goes  to  the  pretectal  area 
and  the  fourth  group  terminates  in  the  superior  col- 
liculus. Figure  i  is  Bishop  &  Clare's  schematization 
of  these  connections.  (See  also  figs.  2  and  3.) 

The  conduction  rates  step  down  by  ratios  of  one 
half  from  group  to  group.  Neither  in  the  frog  nor  the 
cat,  for  example,  can  four  distinct  fiber-size  maxima 
be  demonstrated.  No  qualitative  sensory  differences, 
such  as  are  found  to  be  correlated  with  fiber  size  in 
the  somesthetic  system,  have  yet  been  found  in  the 
visual  system.  Sensation  is  likely  mediated  by  the 
direct  path  to  the  cortex  via  the  large  fiber  group. 

Strong  stimulation  of  the  contralateral  optic  nerve 
elicits  two  definite  spikes,  sometimes  followed  by  a 
prolonged  diminishing  potential  recordable  just  prior 
to  the  dorsal  nucleus  of  the  lateral  geniculate.  The 
second  spike  is  propagated  at  about  one  half  the  rate 
of  the  first,  and  the  threshold  of  its  elicitation  is  about 
•2}/^  times  as  high  as  for  the  first.  Corresponding  to 
these  two  spikes,  there  are  two  postsynaptic  spikes 
manifested  by  the  cells  and  axons  of  the  dorsal  nucleus 
(27,  28). 

Single  shocks  to  the  optic  nerve  induce  complete 
cortical  responses  even  when  such  stimuli  are  weak 
enough  to  activate  onK  the  large-fiber  group,  in- 
ducing the  first  of  the  two  tract  spikes.  The  time  of 
arrival  at  the  cortex  of  the  response  to  the  second 
group  of  radiation  fibers  does  not  tally  with  any  of 
the  prominent  spike  components  of  the  cortical  record. 
Instead,  the  activity  induced  in  layer  B  of  the  genicu- 
late is  propagated  to  the  lateral  nucleus  of  the  thala- 
mus. It  first  emerges  when  stimuli  just  strong  enough 
to  elicit  the  second  spike  are  used,  and  thus  the 
activity  is  not  a  response  induced  there  by  activity 
coming  back  from  the  cortex. 

When  records  are  obtained  from  the  postsynaptic 
elements  in  the  geniculate,  the  response  to  the  first 
tract  spike  from  the  contralateral  eye  arises  mainly 
from  layer  A  and  the  response  to  the  second  spike 
from  layer  B.  Thus  the  ensuing  responses  from  the 
activation  by  the  first  tract  spike  reach  the  cortex, 
and  those  from  the  second  spike  reach  the  thalamus. 
This  represents  a  functional  differentiation  of  the  two 
geniculate  layers.  These  two  lasers  are  also  different 
histologically  (59),  but  we  do  not  yet  know  the  sig- 
nificance of  the  difference. 

The  response  of  the  middle  layer  Ai  to  the  stimu- 
lation of  the  homolateral  optic  nerve  is  mainly  to  the 
first  tract  spike.  When  occasionally  a  second  post- 
synaptic spike  is  elicited,  its  threshold  is  the  same  as 


7IO  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY'    I 


Jt^  ^^'^^^^-.V-'^-'-'V-BP* 


J I IL. 


Fv_l 


1 


FIG.  2.  Postsynaptic  responses  following  single  stimuli  to  the  optic  nerve  from  the  pretectal  area 
(records  i  to  3),  from  the  colliculus  (i?  to  ;o)  and  from  both  (^  to  7).  The  presynaptic  responses 
are  usually  too  low  to  detect.  Time  scale  is  in  10  msec,  intervals.  In  each  column  the  first  record 
is  below  threshold  for  the  major  responses  to  be  recorded  but  nearly  maximal  for  the  second  tract 
spike.  For  record  2,  stimulus  strength  is  1.5  times  that  for  ;,  for  3  it  is  twice.  For  5  to  7,  strengths 
are  3,  5,  and  6  times  that  for  4.  For  g  to  10,  strengths  are  3  and  5  times  that  for  8.  Ratios  of  thresh- 
olds depend  materially  upon  durations  of  square-wave  pulse  stimuli,  the  ratios  increasing  with 
shorter  duration  of  pulses.  First  column:  Records  from  electrodes,  one  at  the  surface  above  pretectal 
area  and  one  below  surface.  The  wave  form  varies  widely  with  different  positions  of  electrodes,  and 
the  spikes  recorded  here  are  not  usually  as  prominent.  Second  column:  Critical  electrode  in  anterior 
border  of  superior  colliculus  where  it  evidently  recorded  activity  both  of  pretectal  character  (with 
latency  of  7  msec,  shown  in  l\\e  first  column)  and  of  colliculus  type  (latency  11  msec,  third  column^ 
Reference  electrode  in  the  medial  geniculate  body.  Note  growth  of  second  potential  in  records  6 
and  7  as  a  wave  starting  beyond  the  crest  of  the  first  (starts  marked  by  vertical  lines)  when  stimulus 
strength  is  increased.  Third  column:  The  second  only  of  the  two  waves  of  record  6  is  recorded  from  a 
critical  electrode  just  below  the  surface  of  the  colliculus  proper  against  a  reference  electrode  at  a 
distance.  Critical  electrode  negative.  Record  8  is  at  higli  amplification,  q  and  10  at  one  quarter  of 
this  amplification.  [From  Bishop  &  Clare  (24).] 


the  first  response.  Layers  A  and  Ai  arc  homologous  in 
function,  both  being  activated  by  the  first  tract  spike 
and  both  relaying  to  the  cortex.  Both  retinas,  how- 
ever, send  fibers  to  each  colliculus,  though  many  more 
arrive  from  the  contralateral  retina  than  from  the 
other  (24,  59). 

Bishop  el  al.  (29)  and  Bishop  &  McLeod  (30)  also 
have  studied  the  response  of  the  lateral  geniculate 
body,  finding  it  repetitive  under  the  conditions  used. 
They  attribute  repetitiveness  to  origins  outside  the 
geniculate,  possibly  to  excitation  over  reverberatory 
circuits  leading  back  from  the  cortex.  Such  paths  have 
not  been  established  anatomically. 

The  responses  of  structures  beyond  the  relay  nuclei 
(the   cortex,    the   lateral    thalamic    nucleus   and    the 


tectum)  differ  from  those  up  to  and  including  the  relay 
nuclei.  Instead  of  being  mainly  spikes,  they  are  ex- 
tended responses  involving  complex  pictures  of  chains 
of  neurons,  each  link  active  in  turn. 

In  the  cat,  it  is  at  present  feasible  to  distinguish 
histologically  only  three  fiber-size  ranges  in  the  optic 
tract.  The  large-size  group  includes  fibers  ranging 
from  8  to  12  /j,  and  these  pass  only  to  the  dorsal 
geniculate  nucleus  and  are  included  in  the  first  of  the 
four  functional  groups  of  fibers  already  mentioned. 
The  middle  histological  group  with  fibers  ranging 
from  4  to  8  |i  also  makes  up  some  of  those  in  the  first 
functional  group  as  well  as  some  in  the  second  spike 
group.  The  small-fiber  group  constitutes  the  fibers  in 
the  third  and  fourth  functional  groups.  Although  the 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION  719 


^.^^ 

5 

^ 

^\^~^.f-»\^A^ 

ffA 

/Vvv>v^ 

FIG.  3.  Responses  from  the  lateral  nucleus  of  the  thalamus  to  the  second  postsynaptic  volley 
from  dorsal  nucleus  of  the  geniculate  with  single  optic  nerve  stimuli.  The  first  record  in  each  column 
was  from  a  stimulus  just  below  threshold  for  the  second  postsynaptic  spike  as  recorded  from  the 
dorsal  nucleus.  Strength  of  stimulus  for  record  2  was  5  times  that  for  / ;  for  4,  3.5  times  that  for  3; 
for  6,  2.5  times  that  for  j.  Record  7  is  a  duplicate  of  (J  at  1.5  and  higher  amplification.  In  the  second 
column  the  cortical  record  appears  on  the  second  oscillograph  beam.  In  the  third  column  the  response 
consists  of  a  sequence  of  brief  spikes.  Time  scale  for  thalamic  records  in  i  o  msec,  intervals.  Latency 
of  response  cannot  be  accurately  determined  but  is  not  over  6  msec.  Form  of  response  varies  widely 
with  location  of  electrodes  and  with  depth  below  the  surface  of  the  thalamus,  and  varies  considerably 
at  one  locus  following  identical  stimuli.  All  records  presented  were  from  a  critical  electrode  in  the 
dorsal  or  dorsolateral  region  of  the  lateral  nucleus  approximately  at  the  level  of  the  anterior  tip 
of  the  dorsal  nucleus  of  the  geniculate.  The  reference  electrode  v/as  deeper  in  the  thalamus  or  in 
white  matter  lateral  to  it.  [From  Bishop  &  Clare  (24).] 


locations  of  the  fiber  groups  in  the  cross  section  of  the 
tract  are  known  (24),  it  is  not  necessary  to  delineate 
them  here. 

CORTICAL  RESPONSE.  The  cortical  response  of  the  cat 
to  a  peripheral  input  as  simple  as  it  is  possible  to 
deliver  is  exceedingly  complex.  The  simplest  pattern 
may  be  shown  by  the  recorded  events  in  the  optic 
cortex  following  single  stimuli  to  the  stump  of  the 
optic  nerve.  The  afiferent  radiation  fillers  conduct 
impulses  mainly  to  the  fourth  layer  of  the  cortex. 
Activity,  of  course,  immediately  spreads  to  the  other 
cortical  layers.  This  is  pictured  in  the  record  as  a 
sequence  of  three  definite  spikes  interpretable  as  indi- 
cating that  three  groups  of  cell  bodies  are  discharging 
in  sequence  (20).  More  intimate  examination  (23)  of 
the  early  part  of  the  response  shows  that  a  second  spike 
sequence  also  occurs.  It,  of  course,  is  less  prominent 
than  the  one  just  mentioned.  In  the  record,  the  second 
series  (the  small  spikes)  alternates  with  the  first.  The 
authors  have  reason  to  infer  that  the  sinall  spikes 
represent  the  short-axon  cells  of  the  cortex.  These 
cells  do  not  possess  long  apical  dendrites  as  do  the 
pyramid  cells.  Their  axons  are  short  and  mingle  with 
the  adjacent  pyramid  cells.  It  is  supposed  that  they 


conduct  activity  from  one  group  of  pyramids  to 
another. 

The  model  of  activity  that  Clare  &  Bishop  (37) 
suggest  is  as  follows  and  is  pictured  in  figure  4.  Af- 
ferent radiation  fibers  first  activate  the  short  axon  cells 
of  the  fourth  layer  of  the  cortex.  These,  in  turn,  in- 
nervate a  group  of  pyramid  cells  at  about  the  same 
cortical  level.  These  cells  discharge  into  their  axons. 
The  main  branches  of  these  leave  this  level  of  the 
cortex  via  the  subcortical  white  matter,  activating 
other  parts  of  the  central  nervous  system.  The  pyra- 
mid-cell axons  possess  recurrent  branches  that  arbor- 
ize within  the  cortex  activating  a  second  group  of 
short  axon  cells.  Tiicse,  then,  activate  a  second  group 
of  pyramidal  cells.  This  alternation  occurs  until  the 
sequence  first  mentioned  has  been  completed.  Since 
the  synaptic  periods  between  each  two  successive 
spikes  is  less  than  i  msec,  the  transmission  is  thought 
to  be  from  axon  to  cell  body. 

When  activation  of  the  pyramidal  cells  is  intense 
enough,  the  dendrites  of  these  cells  are  definitely  in- 
volved. When  so,  they  conduct  their  effects  toward 
their  terminals.  Clare  &  Bishop  (37)  state  that  this 
produces  the  slow  wave  sequence  typical  of  the  re- 
sponse of  the  visual  cortex.   If  stimulation  is  slight. 


720 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


pyramidal  cells  may  become  active  without  activity 
of  their  apical  dendrites  resulting.  They  suggest  the 
possibility  that  in  normal  cortical  iiehavior  dendritic 
activation  is  minor.  Activation  of  dendrites  via  paths 
in  addition  to  the  afferent  radiation  must  also  be  con- 
sidered, although  in  the  present  case  the  radiation 
impulses  form  the  major  component. 

Neurons  e.xert  their  influences  on  their  surrounds 
by  way  of  their  axon  discharges,  but  neurons  may  re- 


no.  4.  Possible  pattfins  oi  activation  of  neurons  in  the  corte.x. 
The  afferent  radiation  axon  entering;  at  right  activates  the 
Golgi  cell  6'i,  the  axon  .-1  of  which  in  turn  activates  the  pyram- 
idal cell  Pi  at  its  cell  body  and  induces  a  brief  spike  response. 
.\  recurrent  branch  of  the  axon  of  Pi  is  shown  activating  G^, 
which  in  turn  activates  P,;  etc.,  accounting  for  the  alternate  high 
and  low  spike  sequences  of  the  response.  Circuits  from  other 
sources  are  able  to  activate  the  dendrites  of  Pi  and  set  up  slow 
wave  responses.  B  is  such  a  circuit  ending  along  the  apical 
dendrite,  C  ending  at  its  terminals  and  D  ending  on  the  basal 
dendrites.  [From  Clare  &  Bishop  (37).] 


ceive  their  effects  either  through  cell  body  synap.ses  or 
through  their  dendritic  synapses.  Activation  through 
cell  body  synapses  has  been  considered  to  require 
more  than  a  single  impulse.  The  required  number 
may  reach  the  cell  body  via  several  branches  of  one 
axon  synap.sing  with  the  given  cell  body,  or  via  several 
axons,  or  several  impulses  via  one  axon.  The  above 
mentioned  authors  deduce  that  when  a  cell  body  is 
intensely  activated,  its  continued  firing  for  a  time 
after  input  has  ceased  depends  upon  the  beha\ior  of 
dendrites.  These  dendrites  were  activated  bv  the  cell 


body  and  now  in  turn  are  reactivating  the  cell  body 
or,  as  we  might  say,  keeping  it  active.  The  dendritic 
contribution  acts  like  a  steady  current  stimulus  to  the 
cell  body. 

What  has  already  been  said  in  describing  the  initial 
spikes  in  the  nerve  has  been  interpreted  as  picturing  a 
sequence  of  activations  from  lower  levels  in  the  cortex 
to  the  surface.  A  slow  surface  positive  wave  associated 
with  the  spikes  is  another  component  of  the  response. 
A  negative  wave  immediately  follows  the  positive  one, 
and  it  is  found  in  the  lower  third  of  the  cortex,  proba- 
bly originating  in  layer  IV.  Prior  to  this  negative 
wave,  there  is  another  negative  wave.  It  occasionally 
shows  up  in  a  normal  record  and  becomes  the  most 
conspicuous  part  of  a  record  under  strychninization  in 
which  case  it  occupies  the  whole  depth  of  the  cortex. 
Not  only  do  the  two  negative  waves  seem  to  have  dif- 
ferent origins,  but  the  late  negative  wave  is  belicxed 
to  arise  from  cells  other  than  those  responsible  for  the 
positive  components  in  the  record.  The  first  negative 
wave  is  attributed  to  conduction  from  cell  bodies  via 
apical  dendrites  toward  the  surface  of  the  cortex. 
When  not  present  in  the  normal  record,  its  absence  is 
a  sign  that  such  conduction  is  not  induced  by  cortical 
stimulation.  In  weakly  strychninized  preparations, 
before  any  detectal:)lc  effect  is  produced  upon  the 
surface  positive  components,  the  response  represented 
by  the  negativity  in  question  is  made  evident.  When 
large  positive  responses  are  induced,  they  are  followed 
immediately  by  the  early  negativity.  Lower  responses 
are  characterized  by  a  delay  between  the  positive  and 
negative  waves.  In  these  records,  the  two  negative 
waves  are  distinguishable. 

Bishop  &  Clare  (20)  interpret  the  early  positive 
wave  as  representing  the  activity  of  the  basal  dendrites 
of  the  neurons  of  which  the  spikes  indicate  the  activity 
of  cell  bodies.  In  figure  5  is  presented  the  diagram 
given  by  Bishop  &  Clare  to  indicate  the  nature  and 
origin  of  the  five  components  of  the  cortical  response 
of  the  cat.  Figure  6  shows  the  findings  of  Bishop  & 
O'Leary  (25)  on  the  rabbit.  In  both  the  cat  and  the 
rabbit,  the  final  component  of  the  response  may 
repeat  several  times  at  the  rate  of  the  alpha  rhythm. 

Chang  &  Kaada  (33)  also  analyzed  the  cortical 
response  to  optic  nerve  stimulation.  The  description 
is  much  like  the  one  we  have  just  given.  Some  of  their 
interpretation  was  different  from  that  of  Bishop's 
laboratory.  The  authors  did  point  out,  however,  that 
it  is  only  the  slow  waves  of  the  various  components 
of  the  response  that  are  reduced  by  agents  affecting 
the  cortex.  This  is  in  line  with  findings  of  Bishop's 
laboratory  over  the  years. 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION  72 1 


CORTEX  UPPER 


CORTEX  MIDDLE 


CORTEX  LOWER 


OPT.  RADIATION 
THALAMUS 


/W 


MSEC 


14  OPT.  NERVE 


FIG.  5.  Left:  Tentative  inferences  concerning  the  origin  of  cortical  responses  drawn  from  experi- 
mental data.  Roman  numerali  at  lejl  indicate  conventional  cortical  layers  and  furnish  a  scale  of  depth. 
Numerals  /  to  J  refer  to  cortical  spikes;  ,-1+  refers  to  underlying  surface-positive  waves;  B—  refers 
to  late  surface-negative  wave  which  appears  to  arise  from  lower  layers  of  cortex,  .4—  represents  the 
early  surface-negative  wave  only  occasionally  seen  well-developed  in  normal  cortex  but  large  under 
strychnine  where  it  becomes  the  most  prominent  potential  clement  of  the  record.  [From  Bishop  & 
Clare  (20).! 

FIG.  6.  Right:  Diagram  of  responses  of  the  optic  pathway  of  the  rabbit.  At  least  four  elements  of 
the  response,  following  the  activation  of  the  optic  nerve,  can  be  distinguished  in  some  records  although 
any  two  adjacent  elements,  each  presumably  complex,  may  be  confluent  in  a  single  response.  The 
last  of  these  four  may  be  repeated  several  times  at  inter\-als  of  about  0.2  sec.  following  a  single 
shock.  There  is  a  discharge  of  the  corticofugal  fibers  during  at  least  the  first  of  these  repetitive  cortical 
discharges  which  appears  to  facilitate  the  thalamic  neurons  to  a  second  discharge  from  the  optic 
nerve.  This  is  indicated  by  the  Ims'  vertical  arrows  pointing  downward.  (Abscissae,  time;  ordinates, 
voltage.)  [From  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (25).] 


Chang  attributes  deflections  2,  3  and  4  in  his  record 
to  the  activities  of  three  different  geniculocortical 
pathways  and  suggests  that  they  may  conduct  the 
respective  impulses  of  trichromatic  vision.  He  uses  to 
support  this  interpretation  the  findings  of  Pieron  (60) 
to  the  effect  that  latencies  for  seeing  the  three  funda- 
mental colors  are  different.  This  is  thought  to  be 
evidence  that  the  impulses  signalling  these  travel  at 
independent  and  different  velocities. 

We  prefer  to  follow  Bishop  &  Clare  (19,  20,  23), 
Clare  &  Bishop  (37)  and  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (25,  26). 
Bishop  &  Clare  (19)  made  it  a  point  to  check  the 
findings  of  Chang  &  Kaada  in  regard  to  the  kind  of 
potentials  found  in  the  geniculocortical  radiation  and 
found  onK  a  single  and  rapidly  conducting  spike. 
When  a  later  tract  spike  resulting  from  use  of  higher 
stimulus  strengths  is  elicited,  it  represents  impulses 
distributed  mainly  to  the  pulvinar,  pretectal  area  and 
colliculus.  Hence  they  conclude  that  all  the  successive 
spikes  up  to  five  in  number,  except  the  first  one,  that 
can  be  recorded  from  the  cortex  represent  groups  of 
neurons  active  within  the  cortex  itself. 


Bishop  &  Clare  (22)  stimulated  the  optic  and 
parietal  cortex  in  cats  at  various  depths  below  the 
surface.  They  found  that  when  the  cortex  is  stimu- 
lated at  the  surface,  the  response  obtained  from  two 
electrodes,  one  at  the  surface  and  the  other  at  any 
depth,  is  a  simple  negative  wave.  When  stimulation  is 
presented  i:)elow  the  surface,  a  diphasic  wave  with  its 
initial  phase  surface-positive  is  obtained.  When 
stimulation  is  presented  half  way  or  more  down 
through  the  cortex,  first  a  single  and  then  two  or 
three  short  spikes  are  manifested  in  the  response.  These 
are  comparable  to  those  elicited  from  activation  of 
the  radiation  pathway.  As  the  radiation  terminals  are 
approached,  the  complete  cortical  response  to  pe- 
ripheral afferent  stimulation  is  simulated.  This  pro- 
cedure is  thus  a  way  of  showing  the  transition  from 
direct  cortical  stimulation  to  the  indirect  or  pe- 
ripheral. 

The  main  difference  between  direct  and  indirect 
stimulation,  in  addition  to  the  possible  simultaneous 
activation  by  the  direct  stimulus  of  elements  that 
respond  successively  to  indirect  stimulation,  pertains 


722 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


.  A^sAAa/\x\^ 


FIG.  7.  Diagrammatic  reconstruction  of  typical  records,  led 
from  surface  to  white  matter,  by  addition  of  what  are  inferred 
to  be  their  chief  potential  components,  a.  Diphasic  response 
assignable  to  conduction  along  apical  dendrites  when  these  only 
are  activated  below  the  surface  of  the  cortex;  negative  phase 
is  early  negative  component,  b.  Summation  of  surface-positive 
potentials  inferred  to  be  caused  by  activity  of  successively 
activated  basal  dendrites  at  successively  higher  levels  of  the 
cortex,  f.  .Spike  sequence  detectable  in  responses  to  near- 
threshold  stimulation  of  the  optic  nerve,  later  responses  of 
which  are  usually  obscured  following  stronger  stimuli,  d.  Slow, 
presumably  diphasic  process  arising  from  deeper  layer  of  cor- 
tex, the  second  phase  of  which  appears  as  a  late  negativity  in 
many  respon.ses.  /.  From  addition  of  all  four  of  above  com- 
ponents, a  reconstruction  of  a  record  of  response  to  optic  nerve 
stimulation  which  shows  considerable  early  negativity.  2. 
Similar  reconstruction  of  response  to  direct  stimulation  fairly 
deep  in  the  cortex,  but  above  layers  V  and  VI,  eliminating 
component  d\  negative  phase  of  diphasic  component  a  is 
exaggerated,  as  appears  to  be  the  case  in  records  from  direction 
stimulation,  and  the  first  spikes  are  telescoped.  3.  Reconstruc- 
tion as  in  ;,  but  for  record  showing  minimal  or  no  early  nega- 
tivity, from  which  therefore  component  a  is  eliminated.  [From 
Bishop  &  Clare  (22).] 


to  the  way  the  apical  dendrites  behave  (see  fig;.  7). 
When  these  structures  are  directly  activated  from  the 
region  of  the  neuron's  cell  bodies,  the  dendrites 
always  conduct  toward  the  surface  of  the  cortex. 
When  indirectly  activated,  by  way  of  the  radiation, 
this  conduction  often  does  not  materialize,  even  fol- 
lowing maximal  optic  ner\e  stimulation.  It  generally 
fails  in  connection  with  very  weak  stimulation.  They 
deduced  that  in  normal  (nonsynchronou.s)  activation 
conduction  does  not  occur  from  cell  ijody  to  apical 
dendrite.  On  the  other  hand,  supposedly  when  a 
sufiicient  number  of  dendrites  are  activated,  some 
kind  of  mutual  facilitation  provides  for  antidromic 
conduction. 

These  authors  believe  that  when  strychnine  or  any 
other  convulsive  drug  is  applied,  the  spike  manifesta- 
tion is  primarily  the  indication  of  a  conducted  re- 
sponse in  apical  dendrites.  The  essence  of  the  convul- 
sive state  lies  both  in  the  heightened  irritability  of  the 
dendrites  and  in  the  mutual  facilitation  and  activa- 
tion to  the  point  of  a  massive  and  well  synchronized 
discharge. 

The  cortical  response,  recorded  from  leads  from 
brain  surface  to  white  matter,  is  a  composite 
(summation)  of  many  primary  sources  of  potential. 
The  response  to  a  single  brief  stimulus  to  the  optic 
nerve  producing  a  volley  of  impulses  may  produce  a 
record  that  is  an  inadequate  representation  of  cortical 
function  in  normal  behaxior.  The  brief  stimulus  pro- 
duces a  degree  of  synchronization  that,  in  itself,  is  an 
artificial  distribution  of  impulses  from  the  start.  This 
sort  of  volley  could  be  considered  more  appropriate 
for  producing  con\ulsions  than  for  the  usual  response 
(23).  One  of  the  justifications  for  this,  however,  aside 
from  procedural  necessity,  is  that  the  normal  ob.ser\er 
can  make  a  visual  discrimination  from  such  stimuli 
which  are  shorter  than  might  ordinarily  be  thought 
efTective.  Actually,  for  certain  comparisons  between 
perceptions  very  short  pulses  of  photic  stimulation  are 
found  usable  and  analytically  helpful.  Of  course, 
none  of  such  stimuli  is  actually  as  brief  as  the  electric 
shocks  used. 

While  the  investigations  of  Bishop  &  Clare  cited  in 
this  chapter  indicate  a  general  propagation  of  mass 
impulse  from  cell  to  cell  upward  from  the  neighbor- 
hood of  the  terminals  of  afferent  fibers  and  apparently 
downward  also  to  lower  cortical  layers,  the  norma! 
stratification  may  be  less  sharp  than  has  been  de- 
scribed. Apparently,  as  more  and  more  minute 
regions  are  explored  with  closely  located  microelec- 
trodes,  an  increasing  heterogeneitv  in  the  directions 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


723 


of  propagated  impulses  becomes  evident.  Marshall 
^55)  has  shown  a  remarkable  degree  of  temporal 
summation  of  the  optic  pathway  to  be  recordable  at 
the  geniculate.  Bishop  &  Clare  (19,  20)  have  done 
likewise  for  the  effects  manifested  at  the  cortex.  The 
latter  authors  have  pointed  out  that,  with  graded 
stimulation,  a  large  effect  reaching  the  geniculate 
neurons  is  required  to  produce  even  a  threshold  re- 
sponse in  them.  Furthermore,  supraliminal  activity 
of  the  afferent  radiation  is  needed  to  produce  a  thres- 
hold response  at  the  corte.x.  Increasing  the  strength  of 
stimulation  upward  from  this  level  prolongs  the  corti- 
cal response  with  its  spikes  and  waves. 

They  also  point  out  that  weak  stimulation  is,  in 
effect,  the  stimulation  that  fails  to  produce  much 
spatial  summation.  In  normal  activity  of  the  optic 
pathway,  this  lack  of  summation  ought  to  be  charac- 
teristic and,  accordingly,  lead  to  incomplete  or  brief 
cortical  responses.  But  the  fact  that  normal  stimula- 
tion is  characteristically  prolonged  rather  than  ab- 
breviated to  a  very  small  fraction  of  a  second,  would 
provide  for  temporal  summation  that  ought  to  com- 
pensate for  the  lack  of  the  spatial  variety.  Relevant  to 
this,  Chang  (32)  has  demonstrated  the  extreme  effec- 
tiveness of  'potentiation'  by  photic  stimulation  of  the 
retina  of  responses  to  individual  brief  stimuli  at  the 
geniculate.  This  steady  photic  stimulation  seemed  to 
maintain  a  raised  level  of  excitation  and  this  made 
incidental  impingements  more  effective.  We  know 
that  exceedingly  weak  excitation  of  the  retina  trans- 
mits something  to  the  cortex.  Not  only  does  this  slight 
effect  get  through  to  the  cortex  but  certain  effects 
from  adjacent  cortical  areas  are  afso  produced  in 
order  that  the  activity  in  the  visual  cortex  be  given 
a  context  that  would  provide  meaning  for  the  terminal 
input.  Bishop  &  Clare  (23)  describe  well  how  the 
experimental  conditions  of  the  laboratory  emphasize 
the  effects  of  spatial  interaction  at  the  expense  of  what 
may  occur  via  temporal  interaction. 

From  their  work  and  knowledge  of  cytoarchitec- 
ture,  Bishop  &  Clare  (23)  depict  the  kind  of  interac- 
tion between  cortical  elements  that  would  plausibh' 
occur.  The  description  is  as  follows.  The  elements  in 
the  cortical  network  can  be  suppo.sed  to  constitute  a 
system  of  both  parallel  and  series  connections.  Each 
afferent  channel  (fiber)  at  any  given  synaptic  level 
would  be  in  connection  with  a  number  of  postsynaptic 
elements,  the  arrangement  involving  definite  over- 
lapping. This  would  be  the  parallel  set  of  connections. 
In  addition  to  this,  elements  at  each  synaptic  level 
send  axons  to  the  next  higher  level,  and  this  provides 


series  connections.  The  authors  suggest  two  further 
features  of  the  system.  Afferents  from  sources  collateral 
to  the  visual  projection  system  surely  connect  at  some 
or  all  levels.  What  they  call  'jumpers'  may  be  in- 
volved. These  are  collaterals  affecting  more  than  a 
single  synaptic  level.  An  example  may  be  the  recurrent 
axons  of  the  pyramidal  cells.  Some  authors  have 
reported  afferent  radiation  fibers  terminating  not  only 
at  the  usual  layer  IV  but  also  at  the  two  successive 
layers  above  it.  Bishop  &  Clare  inject  an  additional 
assumption,  namely,  that  the  mass  of  impulses  tra- 
versing the  one-step  circuits  are  more  effecti\e  than 
those  involving  a  jump  of  two  or  three  synapses. 
Perhaps  the  latter  pathways  should  be  considered  to 
need  assistance  even  to  fire  the  synapses.  The  activity 
just  suggested  is  pictured  in  figure  8,  schematized  in 
figure  9.  The  evidence  for  the  scheme  consists  in  a 
double  series  of  spikes,  ones  of  low  amplitude  alter- 
nating with  ones  of  high  amplitude  when  certain  sub- 
maximal  records  are  obtained  from  potentiometer 
leads  and  from  electrodes  subtending  only  small  frac- 
tions of  the  total  cortical  depth.  The  timing  of  the 
spikes  is  about  i  .4  msec,  between  those  of  the  same 
series,  and  0.7  msec,  between  any  two  of  the  alternate 
series.  Bishop  &  Clare's  suggestion  is  that  this  double 
series  is  made  up  of  pyramidal  cells  alternating  in  dis- 
charge with  short  axon  cells  (Golgi  II).  Since  the 
latter  cells  are  oriented  in  random  fashion,  their 
potentials  would  tend  to  be  registered  by  the  leads  as 
lower  in  amplitude.  The  pyramids  extend  in  a  single 
direction.  This  description  diflfers  from  that  of  Thomas 
&  Jenkner  (66)  in  which  records  were  interpreted  as 
evidencing  repetitive  firing  of  the  same  cells. 

ACTIVATION   OF  CORTEX  BY  STIMULATION   OF  RADIATION. 

Instead  of  initiating  activity  in  the  optic  pathway  by 
stimulating  the  optic  nerve,  it  is  possible  to  eliminate 
the  geniculate  and  stimulate  the  radiation  and  note 
the  cortical  effects.  By  stimulating  at  this  site.  Bishop 
&  Clare  (23)  found  that  the  same  cortical  response 
was  obtainable  as  when  the  cortex  is  stimulated 
through  the  geniculate.  Certain  features  of  cortical 
response  must  be  independent  of  the  geniculate  cycle. 
To  single  stimuli  constant  in  intensity,  the  initial 
cortical  response  spike  attributable  to  radiation  axons 
was  constant  in  amplitude.  Throughout  a  period  of  a 
few  milliseconds,  the  specific  response  to  a  second 
stimulus  manifested  only  slight  diminution  of  ampli- 
tude in  its  positive  pha.se.  The  early  negative  wave  of 
the  second  response  was  depressed  nearly  to  the 
vanishing  point,  and  this  effect  covered  the  whole  de- 


7'24  HANDBOtm    OK    I'HNSIOLOGV  ^  NEHROPHYSIOKOCY    1 


FIG.  8.  Records  of  cat  optic  cortex  to  show  minor  spike  series.  ;.  Total  corte.x,  stimulation  of 
radiation  above  the  geniculate.  2.  Same,  but  stimulation  of  the  optic  nerve;  negativity  failed  to 
dc\elop  in  a  weak  response,  and  between  each  pair  of  spikes  there  occurs  a  minor  disturbance. 
3.  Radiation  stimulus;  potentiometer  balance  to  accentuate  first  minor  spike.  4.  Like  .:?  (in  a  differ- 
ent cat),  a  subma.ximal  response  of  total  cortex  thickness  showing  a  succession  of  spikes  at  half  the 
intervals  of  major  sequence.  5.  Balanced  record  showing  first  and  second  minor  spikes.  6  and  7. 
.Stimulus  at  the  radiation,  different  fractions,  similarly  balanced.  5  to  7.  From  same  cat  as  /  to  3. 
8.  Weak  response  like  4  (from  a  different  cat),  showing  double  sequence.  Major  spike  intervals 
marked  on  4  and  8  were  obtained  from  other  records  of  the  respective  preparations  of  fonn  of 
record  1.  g.  Total  cortex,  submaximal  stimulus  to  radiation,  like  -',  first  and  second  minor  spikes 
recorded.  10.  Balanced  record  accentuating  second  of  these.  11.  Maximal  response,  lower  ampli- 
fication, same  cat.  12.  Total  cortex.  13.  Record  as  in  12  but  balanced  to  accentuate  the  first  and 
second  minor  spikes.  14.  Same  as  /j  but  at  half  the  stimulus  strength  and  recorded  at  twice  the 
amplification.  [From  Bishop  &  Clare  (23).] 


pression  period.  Negativity  representing  antidromic 
conduction  via  apical  dendrites  toward  the  cortical 
surface  appears  to  be  depressed  more  easily  by  prior 
acti\ity  than  the  other  features  of  response. 

In  the  period  of  depression,  the  base  line  slowly 
became  negative  during  which  responses  to  second 
stimuli  were  diminished.  Following  the  depression,  the 
positive  phase  of  the  specific  cortical  responses  to 
second  stimuli  returned  to  normal  amplitude. 

Clare  &  Bishop,  from  these  and  other  results,  con- 
cluded that  most  of  the  features  of  the  cortical  re- 
sponse can  be  attributed  to  the  excitability  cycle  of 
the  cortex  itself.  On  the  basis  of  this  information  on 
the  cortical  cycle  and  Marshall's  (55)  analysis  of  the 
geniculate  cycle,  cortical  responses  to  optic  nerve 
stimulation  were  studied. 

The  degree  of  depression  in  the  geniculate  response 
to  a  second  stimulus  \aries  greatly  from  one  animal 
to  another  as  well  as  to  varying  intensities  of  optic 
nerve  stimulation.  Aside  from  the  possibilitv  that  some 


ol  the  \ariatioii  may  be  due  to  anesthesia  differences, 
the  variability  may  arise  from  the  contextual  or  back- 
grotmd  excitation  upon  which  the  activation  is  super- 
imposed. 

If  two  stimuli  are  delivered  very  close  together,  a 
single  suprama.ximal  response  may  occttr.  Even  when 
an  initial  maxiiual  stimulus  is  invoK'ed,  there  seem  to 
be  a  number  of  elements  that  were  not  activated  but 
only  possibly  excited  subliminalh .  These  can  be 
acti\ated  by  the  second  stimulus,  hence  producing  a 
supermaximal  response  to  the  paired  shocks.  This 
principle  is  more  pronounced  when  the  shocks  are 
submaximal.  The  facilitation  period  soon  ends  and 
beyond  it  a  period  of  depression  ensues.  It  may  last 
as  long  as  5  sec. 

We  may  inject  here  the  idea  that  this  period  may 
possibly  bring  about  perceptual  (\isual)  end  results 
simtilating  adaptation  in  the  retina.  Some  of  the  many 
visual  experiments  labeled  those  of  adaptation  have 
to  do  with  short  term  effects.  The  various  experiments 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


/-'5 


of  Schouten  and  Ornstein,  and  of  Fry  and  colleagues 
are  the  ones  referred  to. 

Clare  &  Bishop  (34)  noted  that  even  during  the 
deepest  depression,  a  second  stimulus  may  find  that 
a  first  one,  though  maximal,  may  have  left  some  ele- 
ments that  were  not  stimulated.  This  is  as  if  at  no  time 
can  any  stimulus  deliverable  to  the  optic  nerve  fire  all 
channels  in  the  radiation  pathway.  The  opposite  ex- 
treme of  this  may  take  place.  A  second  stimulus  may 
cause  a  large  geniculate  response  at  any  instant  fol- 
lowing a  maximal  first  one.  From  this,  they  infer  that 
many,  though  not  all,  elements  represented  in  the  first 
response  are  likewise  involved  in  the  second.  When 
the  second  stimulus  is  made  weaker,  the  second  re- 
sponse manifests  first  a  more  marked  diminution  than 
when  the  first  stimulus  is  applied  alone.  Ultimately, 
the  response  disappears  completely  at  a  stimulus 
strength  otherwise  able  to  elicit  a  large  response  if  not 
preceded  by  an  earlier  stimulus.  If,  now,  the  first 
stimulus  is  diminished  the  response  to  the  second 
grows,  indicating  the  acti\ation  of  elements  not  ac- 
tivated by  the  first  stimulus. 

ACTIVITY     IN     REGIONS     OTHER     THAN     OPTIC     CORTEX. 

Clare  c&  Bishop  (35)  studied  the  activity  of  a  portion 
of  the  lower  half  of  the  medial  wall  of  the  suprasylvian 
gyrus  in  the  cat.  This  is  an  area  responding  only  sec- 
ondarily to  activity  in  the  striate  cortex.  They  re- 
corded from  this  area,  first  to  optic  nerve  stimulation 
and  second  to  direct  electrical  stimulation  of  points  in 
the  striate  area  itself. 

They  found  that  this  region  responds  quite  similarly 
to  the  striate  area,  but  with  an  amplitude  about  one 
eighth  or  less.  The  response  with  all  its  components 
appears  about  i  msec,  later  than  does  the  striate 
response,  when  the  activation  is  induced  by  the 
impulses  in  the  optic  nerve.  The  response  to  striate 
stimulation  is  late  by  only  a  very  short  conduction 
time.  Experimentation  showed  that  this  region  was 
fired  by  the  discharge  of  the  second  major  spike  of  the 
optic  cortex  response  to  optic  nerve  stimulation, 
which  is  to  say,  the  activity  of  the  pyramids  in  layer 
IV  and  probably  in  layer  II.  This  activity  probably 
represents  an  association  area  interrelating  acoustic 
and  optic  activities. 

Jasper  et  al.  (48),  utilizing  repetitive  activation  pro- 
ducing local  convulsive  acti\ity  in  ihe  striate  cortex 
of  the  monkey,  did  not  disclo.se  active  pathways  acro.ss 
the  cortex  from  striate  to  parastriatc  and  other  areas. 
They  did  find  activity  transmitted  to  the  pulvinar. 
When  this  region  is  activated  by  direct  experimental 


m 


'iXTXt 


FIG.  9.  Schematic  diagram  of 
cell  network  suggested  by  analy- 
sis of  spike  responses.  Solid  l,„es: 
A,  afferent  radiation;  B  to  D 
sequence  of  cell  groups.  Differ- 
ences between  short  a.xon  cells 
and  pyramids  are  ignored  in  the 
interest  of  diagrammatic  sim- 
plicity. Dashed  lines:  Collateral 
■jumpers'  impinging  on  cells  fall- 
ing later  in  sequence  than  next 
adjacent  cell.  Dotted  lines:  Affer- 
ents  from  sources  other  than 
geniculate.  Axons  leaving  the 
cortex  are  omitted.  Lines  ending 
blindly  indicate  synaptic  con- 
nections similar  to  those  repre- 
sented. This  fundamental  as- 
sumption is  required  to  render 
the  diagram  functionally  applica- 
ble; insofar  as  impulses  at  syn- 
apses are  equivalent,  a  minimal 
number  (more  than  one)  is  re- 
quired to  fire  a  cell  at  which  they 
arrive  simultaneously.  From  A  simultaneous  impulses  arrive 
at  B  and  C.  B  will  be  activated  if  .-1'  fires  with  A,  and  C  will 
not  until  activated  by  synchronous  firing  of  B  and  B',  or  A  and 
B,  etc.  Activated  by  a  synchronous  volley  of  impulses  from  ra- 
diation fibers,  a  simple  succession  of  synchronized  discharges 
should  ensue.  Activated  by  a  barrage  of  impulses  from  same 
source,  a  much  more  scattered  discharge  should  result,  owing 
to  arrival  of  impulses  at  each  level  over  different  paths  Qsolid 
and  dashed  /m«),  and  with  different  delays.  Arrival  of  im- 
pulses from  other  sources  {dotted  lines')  should  further  mod- 
ulate the  patterns.  [From  Bishop  &   Clare  (23.)) 


stimulation,  it  was  found  to  activate  areas  of  corte.x 
alongside  the  optic  cortex. 

Marshall  et  al.  (57},  on  the  other  hand,  in  exploring 
the  cat's  cortex  found  an  area  of  the  cortex  over- 
lapping with  the  acoustic  cortex.  From  it,  they  were 
able  to  obtain  response  both  to  the  acoustic  and  to 
optic  stimuli.  Bishop  &  Clare  (24)  found  that  the 
relay  fibers  responding  to  the  second  spike  in  the 
optic  tract  terminate  in  the  thalamus. 

Chang  &  Kaada  (33)  interpreted  the  three  spikes 
of  the  cortical  record  following  single  shock  stimula- 
tion as  attributable  to  separate  groups  of  fibers  from 
the  thalamus.  They  assume  that  each  of  the  three 
spikes  is  followed  in  turn  by  a  surface  positive  wave, 
but  that  these  three  longer-lasting  effects  sum  in  the 
record  to  a  single  surface  positive  wave.  Bishop  & 
Clare  (19)  re-examined  the  matter.  To  do  this,  they 
recorded  the  impulses  in  the  radiation  directly,  fol- 
lowing optic  nerve  stimulation,  using  diphasicity  as  a 
criterion  for  propagation  in  radiation  fibers.    It  was 


726 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


from  this  technique  that  they  found  only  one  spike 
conducting  in  the  radiation  from  geniculate  to  cortex 
and  that  this  was  the  relay  from  the  first  spike  in  the 
optic  tract  prior  to  the  geniculate. 

There  are  possibly  three  subcortical  pathways  the 
activities  of  which  directly  or  indirectly  affect  the 
optic  cortex  in  a  demonstrable  way.  There  is,  of 
course,  the  direct  relay  path,  the  optic  pathway  itself; 
the  collateral  path  of  the  brain  stem  to  association 
nuclei  in  the  thalamus  (65);  and  possibly  a  collateral 
circuit  from  thalamic  relay  nuclei  to  others  in  the 
thalamus  to  the  cortex  (38,  47). 

Bishop  &  Clare  (24)  describe  a  bundle  of  fibers 
lea\ing  the  pulvinar  and  terminating  in  the  temporal 
lobe  of  the  cortex.  When  the  optic  nerve  is  stimu- 
lated, a  weak  and  extended  discharge  is  recorded  in 
this  bundle.  The  activation  of  the  bundle  was  pre- 
sumably from  ijrain-stem  or  thalamic  nuclei  re- 
sponding to  optic  tract  activation.  The  exact  origin  of 
the  discharge  is  not  yet  known,  but  its  temporal 
features  tally  with  the  pretectal  or  lateral  nucleus. 

CORTICAL  LOC^Liz.^TiON.  Hartley  (4),  using  photic 
stimulation  of  the  retina  in  which  two  discrete  retinal 
areas  were  tested,  showed  that  one  part  of  the  cortex 
responded  maximally  to  the  activity  in  one  retinal 
area  A,  and  another  part  of  the  optic  cortex  responded 
ma.ximally  to  another  retinal  area  B.  Stimulation  of 
A  would  not  usually  activate  the  cortical  area  re- 
sponding to  B,  and  \ice  versa.  When  any  sign  of 
response  under  these  conditions  was  detected,  it  was 
merely  an  irregular  train  of  indefinite  waves. 

When  A  and  B  were  stimulated  simultaneously, 
the  corresponding  cortical  areas  did  not  manifest  any 
summation.  The  responses  were  simply  of  the  usual 
size.  On  the  other  hand,  when  the  two  stimuli  were 
separated  by  an  interval  of  150  to  175  msec,  the 
respon.se  to  the  second  was  enhanced;  and  if  the  inter- 
val was  lengthened,  a  value  could  be  found  at  which 
inhibition  or  depression  of  the  second  response  was 
manifested. 

At  a  retinal  point  C,  intermediate  between  A  and 
B,  responses  to  both  stimuli  were  recordable,  both 
responses  being  discernible  when  enough  temporal 
separation  was  allowed  for  the  two  response  waves  to 
be  seen  in  two  parts  of  the  record.  When  simultaneous, 
the  record  representing  the  response  to  the  two  retinal 
areas  was  a  single  wave  larger  than  either  of  the  two 
responses  individually  recorded. 

In  one  experiment,  for  instance,  cortical  point  C 
later  came  to  respond  only  to  the  stimulation  of  the 


second  retinal  area.  At  first,  the  two  stimuli  produced 
an  enhanced  result  when  simultaneous.  When  the 
two  were  separated  by  about  15  msec,  the  entire 
response  almost  disappeared.  This  continued  to  be 
the  case  as  the  separation  was  made  considerably 
greater.  It  did  not  matter  which  of  the  two  stimuli 
was  delivered  first.  When  finally  the  first  retinal  area 
failed  to  produce  a  response  of  its  own,  it  still  could 
augment  B,  when  applied  simultaneously,  and  reduce 
the  size  of  B,  when  definitely  out  of  phase  with  it.  Still 
other  examples  of  the  interaction  of  cortical  areas 
were  obtained. 

Since  this  material  relates  not  only  to  cortical  locali- 
zation but  also  to  the  visual  experience  of  movement, 
it  will  lie  discussed  in  the  section  on  that  subject. 

PROPERTIES  OF  DENDRITES.  Stimulation  of  the  cortex 
at  various  depths  has  led  Clare  &  Bishop  (36,  37)  to 
make  certain  inferences  about  the  behavior  of  cortical 
dendrites.  These  were  given  in  the  section  devoted  to 
the  cortical  response.  The  following  is  a  statement  of 
the  picture  they  paint  of  the  behavior  of  the  various 
parts  of  the  neuron.  In  referring  to  intercortical  paths 
that  terminate  only  on  apical  dendrites  of  pyramidal 
cells,  they  found  cases  in  which  only  the  terminal  por- 
tions near  the  cortical  surface  are  activated.  When 
the  dendrites  are  activated  indirectly  by  way  of  these 
paths,  or  directly  by  artificial  stimuli,  dendritic  con- 
duction is  not  of  the  all-or-none  type.  The  conduction 
occurs  more  readily  away  from  the  cell  body  than  in 
the  reverse  direction.  Its  rate  is  less  than  i  m  per  sec. 

After  initial  indirect  activation,  a  stimulus  finds  the 
dendrites  activatable  at  all  times  later  than  the  abso- 
lutely refractory  period  of  the  axons  involved.  Follow- 
ing a  20  msec,  facilitation  period,  depression  sets  in 
and  the  sign  is  positive.  The  authors  inferred  that,  in 
general,  the  depression  following  neuron  activation  is 
attributable  mainly  to  its  dendrites.  Apical  dendrites 
manifest  no  refractoriness,  and  so  later  activation  sums 
with  the  first.  Continuous  negativity  may  be  perpetu- 
ated by  repetitive  stimulation.  All  that  would  be  re- 
quired to  produce  waves  of  potential  of  any  temporal 
proportions  would  be  modulation  of  stimulation  of  the 
dendrites.  The  authors  suggest  this  principle  in  the 
production  of  the  waves  of  the  cortical  record. 

The  influence  of  a  neuron  on  its  surrounds  is 
brought  about  only  through  the  impulses  it  causes  to 
be  discharged  by  the  cell  body  into  its  axon.  These 
effects  are  variously  distributed  via  the  ramification 
of  axon  branches.  Activation  of  the  neuron  may  occur 
via  two  avenues.  The  one  is  by  way  of  cell-body 
synapses;  the  other  by  way  of  dendrite  synapses.  These 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


727 


two  avenues  ought  to  affect  the  pattern  of  axonal  dis- 
charge from  the  cell. 

The  conditions  applying  to  activation  of  the  neuron 
via  cell-body  synapses  are  as  follows.  More  than  one 
impulse  must  be  delivered  to  the  cell  body  in  order 
to  activate  it.  When  the  discharge  is  once  set  up,  it  is 
of  the  all-or-none  type.  The  impulses  required  for  cell- 
body  activation  may  arrive  via  a  single  axon  branch, 
or  via  several  of  them,  provided  that  they  arrive 
within  the  required  time  limits. 

As  just  implied,  when  once  set  into  action,  the  cell 
body  discharges  once  or  more  without  receiving  fur- 
ther activation.  The  authors  (36,  37)  infer  that,  when 
once  a  cell  body  is  .set  into  action,  its  action  may  be 
sustained  by  its  own  dendritic  activity.  They  say  that 
the  cell  body  once  sufficiently  activated  can  activate 
its  own  dendrites,  and  they  in  turn  can  sustain  activity 
in  the  cell  body.  The  duration  of  the  dendritic  impulse 
seems  to  be  of  the  order  of  15  msec.  This  length  of 
time  provides  for  the  dendrites  acting  like  a  steady- 
current  stimulus  to  the  cell  body  while  it  builds  and 
discharges  .several  times. 

In  cases  of  stimulation  of  dendrites  only,  the  den- 
drites do  not  exhibit  all-or-none  conduction  to  the 
cell  body  which,  therefore,  would  not  be  expected  to 
be  activated.  It  is  presumed  that  some  effect,  never- 
theless, would  be  exerted  by  dendrites  upon  the  cell 
body  by  reason  of  the  excited  state  of  one  and  un- 
excited  state  of  the  other.  The  various  excitations 
induced  in  dendrites  would  sum.  The  authors  sup- 
pose it  proble  that,  if  the  dendrites  are  excited  from 
enough  converging  sources,  they  might  begin  to  con- 
duct and  activate  cell  bodies.  The  role  of  the  dendrite 
seems  to  be  to  rai.se  the  level  of  excitation  of  the  cell 
body  and  thereby  lower  its  threshold  to  influences 
arriving  via  axons  impinging  on  it.  The  chief  charac- 
teristic of  dendritic  action  is  its  graded  character  in 
contrast  to  the  all-or-none  manifestations  of  cell  body 
and  axon.  This  provides  for  a  great  deal  moie  flexi- 
bility and  variety  in  action  than  a  system  limited  to 
all-or-none  activity. 


VISUAL  PHENOMENA  TO  BE  EXPLAINED 

Gross  Response  to  Gross  Intensity  Relations 

One  of  the  major  considerations  in  the  study  of  vision 
and  its  mechanism  is  the  question  of  what  characteris- 
tics of  vision  require  the  striate  cortex  and  what  charac- 
teristics are  demonstrable  when  the  cortex  is  removed. 
It  has  been  found  that  in  some  animals  response  to 
gross  intensitv  relations  in  the  stimulus  field  are  re- 


acted to  in  the  absence  of  the  occipital  cortex.  This 
is  not  at  all  surprising  in  the  light  of  what  we  know 
about  the  bifurcation  of  the  optic  pathway,  some 
fibers  going  to  cortex  and  some  going  to  motor 
centers  that  are  subcortical,  and  in  the  light  of  our 
findings  on  pupillary  responses  which  parallel  Fech- 
ner's  paradox  (see  the  subsequent  section  on  bilateral 
functions).  It  would  seem  possible  that  the  structuring 
of  response  to  gross  flux  differences  could  occur  in  the 
motor  sphere  and  in  the  sensory  spheres  somewhat 
parallel  to  each  other,  according  to  our  interpretation 
of  the  parallelism  described  in  connection  with  Fech- 
ner's  paradox.  If  one  of  the  two  channels  were  to  be 
destroyed,  the  other  might  be  able  to  mediate  an  end 
result.  With  the  cortical  channels  destroyed,  motor 
behavior  of  some  effective  kind  might  still  be  able  to 
be  exhibited.  With  the  motor  channel  destroyed,  one 
could  experiment  only  on  man,  for  he  alone  could  tell 
whether  experiential  reactions  to  intensive  features 
of  stimulation  were  altered.  We  should  not  expect 
them  to  be  in  gross  situations. 

The  following  are  some  of  the  characteristics  of  the 
ijehavior  of  monkeys  in  response  to  visual  stimuli 
when  their  occipital  cortices  are  removed,  eliminating 
the  geniculostriate  systems,  as  described  by  Kliiver 
(50).  In  such  animals,  the  eyelid  reflex  to  photic 
stimulation  is  abolished  permanently.  The  pupillary 
reflex  to  photic  stimulation  is  retained,  however.  The 
sudden  appearance  of  a  stationary  or  moving  photic 
source  does  not  elicit  a  turning  of  the  head  or  eyes 
towards  it,  although  movements  of  the  head  and  eyes 
are  elicited  by  nonphotic  stimuli. 

Conjugate  movements  of  the  eyes  are  not  destroyed; 
neither  does  destruction  of  the  superior  coUiculi 
abolish  such  movements  in  response  to  stimulation  of 
the  cortical  eye  fields.  Visual  placing  reactions  are 
lost.  Nevertheless,  animals  rarely  bump  into  objects 
when  they  are  left  to  themselves  or  are  not  excited. 

When  the  bilaterally  decorticate  monkey  is  in  a 
limited  familiar  habitat,  its  behavior  in  jumping, 
swinging  and  climbing  is  so  readily  executed  that 
the  unsuspecting  observer  would  suppose  the  animal 
to  be  normal.  Variation  in  the  position  of  some  fa- 
miliar object  in  its  cage  elicits  considerable  fumbling 
until  the  animal's  hands  come  into  contact  with  it. 

Such  a  monkey  can  respond  discriminatively  to  the 
more  or  less  intense  of  two  photic  sources  whether  they 
are  indefinitely  present  orappear  suddenly, or  whether 
their  presentation  is  simultaneous  or  successive.  Re- 
sponses to  weak  photic  stimuli  have  demonstrated 
that  the  ai:)solute  threshold  in  the  occipitally  operated 


728 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


90 


80 


60 


m 

_i 

(9 

Z 
1 


1 
«40 


20 


\ 

\ 

\ 

V"? 

,^'' 

V 

V 

^ 

-^ 

. 

30  MSEC 


40 


50 
IMPLICIT   TIME 


60 


70 


FIG.  lo.  The  relation  between  implicit  time  of  the  rabbit's 
cortex  and  the  visual  angle  of  the  target.  Note  break  in  curves 
at  or  near  20  degrees.  This  suggests  that  the  increase  in  target 
size  beyond  this  point  does  not  involve  further  spatial  summa- 
tion at  the  retina,  and  that  further  reduction  in  implicit  time 
is  a  continuation  of  the  effect  from  increasing  the  intensity  of 
incidental  stray  illumination  of  the  retina.  [From  Hartley  (3).] 


monkey  is  not  essentially  different  from  that  of  the 
normal  monke\-  or  of  man. 

The  decorticated  monkey  responds  differentially  to 
two  equally  intense  and  equally  large  targets  if  their 
distances  from  the  eye  are  quite  different.  This  is  to 
say,  the  animal  is  able  to  use  differences  in  total  flu.x 
as  a  factor.  This  reaction,  like  the  others  in  which 
familiarity  with  the  stimulus  is  ab.sent,  has  to  be 
learned  rather  than  immediately  apprehended. 

Responses  to  targets  differing  in  shape,  preopera- 
tively  established,  are  lost  through  the  remosal  of  the 
occipital  lobes.  Neither  can  new  differential  responses 
be  learned  if  the  new  targets  are  compact  and  do  not 
differ  greatly  in  any  dimensional  respect.  A  decorti- 
cate animal,  taught  to  respond  to  a  target  having  a 
greater  amount  of  flux,  will  continue  to  do  so  regard- 
less of  whether  the  flux  is  continuous  or  intermittent. 
Thus  a  source  with  an  on-off  rate  of  4  per  sec.  is 
equivalent  to  a  continuous  one  just  so  long  as  the 
total  flux  per  second  is  the  same. 

Color  vision  is  permanently  lost  in  the  decorticate 
animal.  Kliiver  would  say,  from  his  many  studies, 
that    brightness    vision    is   destroyed    in    the    animal 


without  occipital  cortices.  What  such  an  animal  does 
is  to  respond  on  the  basis  of  total  flux. 

Area  of  Target 

When  a  photic  source  (target)  subtending  some 
known  \isual  angle  at  the  eye  is  used  as  a  stimulus,  the 
resulting  response  of  an  animal  may  be  that  which  we 
could  call  the  perception  of  brightness,  or  it  may  be 
simply  the  response  to  the  stimulus  as  total  flux. 
Brightness  is  the  response  to  flux  per  unit  area  of 
target,  and  may  be  expressed  as  an  experience  or  as 
a  differential  motor  response  on  the  i^asis  of  flu.x  per 
tmit  area.  It  is  not  difficult  to  determine  which  of  the 
two  possible  responses  is  being  made  when  the  human 
is  a  subject,  and  not  too  difficult  when  certain  sub- 
human species  are  being  tested.  It  is  a  little  more 
uncertain  when  inferred  in  some  cases  such  as  from 
the  cortical  response  to  photic  stimulation  of  the 
retina. 

The  experiments  of  Hartley  (3)  on  manipulating 
the  target  area  to  measure  the  implicit  time  of  the 
cortical  response  will  be  presented  here.  Implicit  time 
is  the  time  elapsing  between  the  beginning  of  stimulus 
and  the  peak  of  the  initial  large  wave  of  response.  In 
this  investigation,  target  area  was  varied  from  i  to  go 
degrees  of  visual  angle.  Throughout  this  range  incre- 
ments in  visual  angle  reduced  implicit  time  of  the 
cortical  response.  One  of  the  significant  findings  was 
that  the  relation  between  target  area  and  implicit 
time  was  not  a  simple  function  throughout  the  range 
used  as  was  the  function  of  target  intensity.  To  explain 
this,  it  was  pointed  out  that  the  image  of  the  target 
on  the  retina  was  not  the  only  site  of  retinal  stimula- 
tion. The  retina  as  a  whole  received  stray  radiation 
as  well  as  focused  radiation.  Thus  the  cur\e  showing 
the  relation  between  implicit  time  and  stimulus  area 
is  a  composite  of  the  increasing  spatial  summation 
within  the  image  and  the  increasing  intensits'  of  stray 
photic  flux  on  the  retina  as  a  whole. 

If  the  target  area  operates  as  just  indicated,  one 
ought  not  to  expect  the  resulting  curves  relating  area 
to  implicit  time  and  intensity  to  implicit  time  to 
coincide.  .Since  spatial  interaction  and  stimulus  in- 
tensity are  both  varied  when  target  area  is  manipu- 
lated and  stimulus  intensity  alone  when  photic  in- 
tensity is  manipulated,  the  curve  for  the  former  would 
lie  to  the  left  of  the  latter.  Thus,  if  interaction  (spatial 
summation)  would  operate  over  part  of  the  range  of 
target  manipulation  and  not  over  the  whole  range, 
then  a  break  in  the  response  curve  would  be  expected. 
This  is  what  resulted,  as  appears  in  figure  10.  In  all 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


729 


experiments  in  which  the  target  was  increased  beyond 
about  20  degrees,  a  break  in  the  curve  appeared.  The 
question  of  how  far  across  the  retina  spatial  summa- 
tion may  operate  has  been  dealt  with  by  Adrian  and 
others.  To  say  the  least,  spatial  summation  has  come 
to  be  considered  to  possess  definite  limitations.  The 
limiting  subtense  in  this  investigation  seemed  to  be  in 
the  region  of  20  degrees.  The  curve  in  each  experi- 
ment shows  that  when  targets  of  broad  angular  sub- 
tense are  reduced  in  size,  the  implicit  time  is  length- 
ened. This  continues  until  the  target  is  reduced  to 
about  20  degrees  and  then,  fairly  quickly,  implicit 
time  becomes  much  shorter  than  expected  for  further 
target  area  reductions. 

While  the  present  investigation  offered  no  way  to 
eliminate  the  stimulation  out.side  the  retinal  image,  it 
did  present  evidence  of  the  operation  of  two  stimulus 
components  (increase  in  image  area  and  increase  in 
stray  radiation  intensity).  The  same  evidence  demon- 
strated that  the  two  factors  operated  at  different  rates. 
In  the  demonstration  that  two  factors  were  in  opera- 
tion, the  author  showed  intensity  per  unit  area  of 
retinal  image  was  involved  in  determining  implicit 
time.  Thus  it  can  be  said  it  was  not  merely  flux  as 
such  that  produced  the  response  as  recorded  but  that 
the  response  was  in  a  way  a  brightness  response. 
While  it  was  not  doubted  that  the  rabbit  has  bright- 
ness \ision,  it  was  a  question  from  the  beginning  as 
to  whether  it  could  be  demonstrated  by  the  neuro- 
physiological  experiments  of  the  kind  being  per- 
formed. 


Bnglitness 

The  experience  of  brightne.ss  or  a  motor  response 
based  on  the  same  principle  is  something  different 
than  the  response  to  mere  flux  differences  in  two 
major  portions  of  the  photic  field.  Brightness  is  the 
result  of  manipulation  of  intensity  per  unit  area  of 
visual  target.  Hence  an  area  can  be  seen  as  brighter 
than  another  even  though  the  total  flux  of  the  first 
area  is  less  than  that  of  the  second.  This  would  be  the 
case  if  the  flux  per  unit  area  were  greater.  Kliiver's 
monkeys,  devoid  of  the  geniculostriate  system,  gave 
no  evidence  of  being  able  to  do  this.  They  could  learn 
to  distinguish  between  two  equal-sized  and  equally 
intense  targets  when  one  was  removed  to  a  greater 
distance  than  the  other,  in  which  case  its  retinal 
image  was  smaller.  A  lesser  total  flux  on  the  retina 
for  it  than  for  the  near  target  was  thus  invoked. 
Kliiver  states  that  he  does  not  see  any  evidence  in  the 
behavior  of  cats  and  other  animals  that  would  indicate 


that  they  can  distinguish  brightness  at  subcortical 
levels. 

Electrophysiological  experimentation  upon  the  re- 
sponse of  the  optic  pathway  has  not  been  of  such  a 
nature  as  to  make  the  needed  distinctions  between 
response  to  total  flux  and  to  flux  per  unit  area.  One 
way  to  extract  evidence  on  this  point  is  to  try  to  com- 
pare certain  perceptual  responses  with  the  electro- 
physiological ones  and  make  what  deductions  we  can. 

It  now  seems  as  though  certain  comparisons  be- 
tween brightness  in  perception  and  the  amplitude  of 
the  response  to  specific  brief  stimulation  can  be  made. 
In  a  later  section  we  deal  with  brightness  enhance- 
ment. In  it  we  are  comparing  the  experience  that  is 
evoked  by  steady  continuous  stimulation  with  one 
that  is  evoked  by  intermittent  stimulation.  So  long  as 
we  keep  the  two  areas  equal,  certain  justifiable  com- 
parisons between  the  intensity  needed  in  both  cases 
to  produce  equally  bright  surfaces  can  be  made. 
Many  of  the  conditions  for  producing  the  various 
levels  of  effectiveness  of  the  intermittent  stimuli  seem 
to  be  the  very  same  as  tho.se  similarly  varying  the 
amplitude  of  the  cortical  response  to  such  stimuli. 

Since  continuous  steady  stimulation  produces  noth- 
ing in  the  extended  record  of  cortical  activity,  the 
amplitude  of  which  we  can  measure,  we  are  prevented 
from  making  the  same  amplitude-brightness  compari- 
sons for  steady  photic  impingements.  With  the  con- 
crete evidence  at  present  available,  we  seem  unable  to 
go  beyond  the  gross  comparison  just  described.  Per- 
haps we  do  not  know  enough  regarding  the  measure- 
ment of  steady  states  and  the  relation  of  steady  states 
in  one  part  of  the  cortex  to  those  in  others.  Steady 
states  seem,  at  the  present  state  of  our  knowledge,  to 
be  quite  dead  and  processless.  To  explain  some  things, 
however,  they  seem  to  be  just  what  is  required.  (For 
further  discussion  of  cortical  response  to  continued 
peripheral  stimulation,  see  the  later  section  on  bright- 
ness enhancement.) 

Flicker  and  Fusion 

When  a  series  of  photic  pulses  is  delivered  to  the 
retina,  the  experience  is  flicker,  except  when  the  rate 
of  delivery  reaches  a  critical  value.  Obviously,  the 
intact  human  and  even  certain  subhuman  species  can 
distinguish  between  an  intermittent  and  a  steady 
photic  source.  This  is  true  at  least  down  to  arthropods 
and  crustaceans.  How  the  discrimination  is  accom- 
plished must  differ  in  detail  at  the  various  phylo- 
genetic  levels.  It  would  seem  from  Kliiver's  observa- 
tions that  a  monkey,  clepri\-ed  of  the  geniculocortical 


73" 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROFHYSIOLOGY    1 


apparatus,  can  not  distinguish  between  a  steady 
source  and  one  the  intermittency  of  which  is  as  low  as 
4  cps,  just  so  long  as  the  total  flux  per  unit  time  is  the 
same  in  the  two  cases.  Hence,  although  much  has 
been  said  about  the  photochemical  basis  for  flicker 
and  its  elimination,  the  ultimate  crucial  point  of  de- 
termination of  critical  flicker  frequency  (c.f.f.)  appears 
to  be  in  the  cortex. 

Durinc  flicker,  the  activity  in  the  optic  nerve  waxes 
and  wanes  with  sufficient  amplitude  and  at  such  rates 
that  the  cortical  activity  may  also  vary  in  its  temporal 
aspects  in  significant  ways.  It  has  been  noted  that 
whereas  the  response  of  the  optic  pathway  up  to  and 
including  the  postsynaptic  elements  in  the  lateral 
geniculate  body  are  ijrief  and  spike-like,  response 
beyond  this  is  somewhat  extended  in  time  and  in- 
volves certain  complexities  absent  in  its  precursors. 
This  in  itself  would  be  a  kind  of  e\'idence  for  believing 
that  the  cortex  cannot  respond  at  the  same  high  rate 
as  the  peripheral  mechanisms. 

Be  this  as  it  may,  a  rate  can  be  attained  that  re.sults 
in  the  perception  of  uniform  continuous  light.  The 
point  at  which  this  is  reached  (c.f.f.)  is  also  known  as 
the  fusion  point.  All  rates  above  this  maintain  fusion. 
This  means  that  at  the  fusion  point  any  temporal 
undulations  in  cortical  activity  that  may  occur  are  so 
slight  as  to  be  of  no  ultimate  effect. 

Talbot  found  that  when  fusion  was  reached,  the 
level  of  perceived  brightness  of  the  light  field  was  less 
than  for  a  continuous  and  uniform  stimulus  of  the 
same  intensity.  The  effect  is  as  if  the  input  instead  of 
being  intermittent  were  uniform  and  spread  evenly 
throughout  the  cycle.  Thus,  if  the  PCF  (pulse-to- 
c)cle-fraction)  is  one-half,  the  level  of  brightness  is 
one-half.  Whereas  those  devoted  to  photochemistry 
have  shown  how  this  effect  might  be  attributed  to  the 
manner  in  which  photochemical  systems  react  to 
photic  impingements,  certain  features  of  the  behavior 
of  the  optic  pathway  have  been  overlooked.  One  of 
these  is  the  way  the  neuroretina  behaves.  It  rearranges 
the  temporal  distribution  of  the  sense-cell  discharge 
effects  of  the  retina.  Since  we  are  not  dealing  pri- 
marily with  peripheral  respon.ses,  we  cannot  go  into 
this  matter  further.  Needless  to  say,  the  cortex  must 
take  a  hand  in  even  the  determination  of  critical 
flicker  frequency  and  the  Talbot  effect  (7,  8). 

Since  the  Talbot  effect  represents  the  simplest 
possible  smoothing-out  result  from  a  waxing  and 
waning  stimulus,  we  can  suppose  that  the  cortex 
operates  on  the  simplest  principle  in  that  respect. 

The  following  investigations  in  which  cortical  re- 
sponse was  elicited  by  stimulation  of  the  retina  rather 


than  electrical  stimulation  of  ilic  optic  nerve  was  used 
to  give  some  information  relative  to  the  mechanisms 
at  work  in  flicker  and  fusion.  Bartley  (3-5)  measured 
the  latency  of  the  cortical  response  to  various  forms  of 
photic  stimulation.  One  of  the  factors  varied  was  the 
duration  of  a  "dark'  interval.  When  these  intervals 
were  very  short,  the  off-response  to  the  termination  of 
the  photic  pulse  and  the  on-response  to  the  beginning 
of  the  succeeding  pulse  were  both  evident  in  the 
record  when  the  interval  was  as  short  as  1 2  msec. 
When  this  interval  was  shorter  than  the  implicit  time 
of  the  ofl'-response,  the  resumption  of  stimulation 
did  not  preclude  the  appearance  of  the  off-response, 
nor  the  appearance  of  the  on-response  to  the  begin- 
ning of  the  next  pulse.  Since  12  msec,  compare  to  the 
interval  between  pul.ses  when  pulse  frequency  is  40 
per  sec,  if  the  pulse-to-cycle  fraction  is  one-half,  it 
would  seem  as  though  under  the  conditions  dealt 
with,  Bartley  was  reaching  the  point  called  critical 
flicker  frequency  in  hmnan  flicker  experiments. 

The  implicit  times  of  the  on-  and  off-responses  are 
not  equivalent.  It  would  .seem  from  the  results  (4,  5) 
that  for  similar  conditions  the  implicit  times  of  the 
on-response  are  shorter  than  those  for  the  off-response. 
Thus,  as  the  'dark'  interval  in  the  cycle  is  made 
shorter  and  shorter,  the  off-response  to  the  termina- 
tion of  the  one  pulse  and  the  on-response  to  the  be- 
ginning of  the  succeeding  pulse  finally  becomes  con- 
current. This  might  be  one  factor  iit  ijringing  about 
fusion  in  flicker  experiments,  since  in  some  way  these 
two  responses  might  counteract  each  other  at  some 
final  level  in  the  cortex. 

That  the  two  forms  of  response  (on  and  off)  could 
be  concurrent  is  to  be  understood  from  the  finding  of 
Bartley  that  the  two  responses  occupy  separate  chan- 
nels all  the  way  from  the  retina  to  the  cortex.  One  of 
the  evidences  for  this  was  the  finding  that  an  on- 
response  can  follow  an  off-response  as  clo.sely  as  1 2 
or  fewer  msec,  whereas  an  on-response  to  a  second 
stimulus  cannot  follow  unless  the  two  are  at  least  80 
msec,  apart.  Electrograms  of  the  retina  have  been 
interpreted  as  showing  that  a  second  pulse  presented 
shortly  following  the  termination  of  the  first  will 
inhibit  the  off-response  to  the  first.  Bartley  (4)  showed 
in  a  number  of  ways  that  phenomena  that  were  de- 
tectable in  the  cortical  record  are  not  discernible  in 
the  electroretinogram  recorded  under  the  same  con- 
ditions. It  would  thus  seem  logical  to  rely  on  the 
cortical  record  in  cases  where  differential  responses  in 
the  electroretinogram  fail  to  show  up. 

Bartley  also  measured  the  implicit  time  of  cortical 
on-re.sponse  when  duration  was  the  variable  (2)  and 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION  yjl 


,0N     OFF 


»- 
CO 


en 


> 

•- 
o 


u 

> 


< 
-I 
til 
e 


'PHOTIC   PULSE 


I   SEC 


200% 
100% 

Fig.12    > 

^ 

X 

\   BRIGHTNESS  OF    STEADY    SOURCE 

50% 

B                            ^^^^^ 

o 

0 

PHOTIC    PULSES   PER  SECOND 

0 

10 

20                                          30 

40 

FIG.  II.  The  cortical  response  of  the  rabbit  to  intermittent  stimulation  of  the  eye  in  which  the 
photic  pulse  occupied  one  quarter  of  the  cycle.  Note  responses  both  to  onset  and  to  termination 
of  the  photic  pulse.   [From  Bartley  (9).] 

FIG.  12.  Brightness  enhancement,  the  greater  relative  effectiveness  of  intermittent  stimulation 
than  of  steady  stimulation  at  lovkf  photic  pulse  rates.  This  is  shown  in  curve  A  but  not  in  curie  B. 
B  shows  the  effect  under  some  conditions  of  weak  stimulation.   [From  Bartley  (7).] 


when  area  of  the  target  was  a  variable  (3).  For  varia- 
tion in  duration  of  photic  pulse  to  affect  implicit  time, 
it  must  be  as  short  as  4  or  5  msec,  for  targets  with 
luminosities  as  great  as  2400  candles  per  square  foot, 
and  which  subtend  6  or  7  degrees.  Increasing  area 
reduced  implicit  time  and  thus  would  be  expected 
to  work  in  the  direction  of  raising  critical  flicker 
frequency. 

Jasper  (46)  recorded  potentials  from  the  occipital 
cortex  in  man  following  pulse  rates  of  55  to  60  per  sec, 
and  thus  was  near  the  critical  flicker  frequency  under 
the  conditions.  This  was  not  interpreted  as  being  a 
demonstration  of  driving  the  cortical  alpha  rhythm 
beyond  its  normal  8  to  1 3  per  sec.  frequency.  He  found 
that  the  amplitude  of  the  waves  at  20  per  sec.  was 
about  one-half  of  what  it  was  at  10  per  sec,  and  waves 
at  40  per  sec.  were  about  one-fourth  as  high  as  those 
at  10  per  sec.  A  further  \ery  crucial  observation  also 
supporting  his  interpretation  of  the  impossibility  of 
driving  the  cortex  was  the  following.  As  the  stimulus 
rate  was  slowlv  increased,  there  were  stages  at  which 
the  waves  would  undergo  what  he  called  desynchroni- 
zation.  For  example,  at  frequencies  of  from  14  to  15 
per  sec,  this  would  happen  and  the  result  would  in- 


clude a  shift  in  amplitude  so  that  at  from  18  to  20 
per  sec,  the  amplitude  would  drop  to  one-half  the 
height  up  to  that  time. 

Halstead  and  colleagues  (44,  45,  67)  reported  that 
although  the  dominant  brain  waves  (alpha)  in  the 
monkey  could  be  'driven'  up  to  rates  comparable  to 
critical  flicker  frequency  only,  the  pathway  prior  to 
the  cortex  could  follow  input  intermittencies  beyond 
the  c.f.f.  Obviously,  the  records  of  Halstead  and  col- 
leagues manifest  waves  at  the  rates  indicated  and 
thus,  in  essence,  tally  with  those  of  Jasper.  Whether 
this  is  driving  depends  upon  one's  definition  of  the 
term.  A  standard  definition  has  not  yet  been  put  into 
the  literature. 

The  foregoing  tallies  with  Hartley's  observations 
and  his  alternation  of  response  theor\  (which  is  con- 
sidered in  the  section  on  brightness  enhancement). 
For  example,  one  observation  (3)  was  that  if  the  rate 
of  intermittent  retinal  stimulation  suddenly  delivered 
to  the  retina  was  definitely  above  5  per  sec.  (the  rab- 
bit's alpha  rate),  the  following  would  occur.  A  large 
cortical  response  to  the  first  pulse  would  appear.  No 
response  to  the  second  pulse  would  result.  Then,  the 
responses  to  the  following  few  pulses  would  wax  and 


732 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


wane  or  be  entirely  absent  in  random  order.  Finally, 
all  pulses  would  be  responded  to  by  equal  waves  but 
of  a  reduced  size,  the  amplitude  in  keeping  with  the 
rate.  If  the  rate  was  high  the  amplitude  would  be  low, 
and  vice  versa.  This  irregular  initial  period  was 
looked  upon  as  a  reorganization  period  during  which 
redistribution  of  the  various  retina-to-cortex  channels 
responsive  to  successive  stimuli  was  brought  about  so 
that  all  stimuli  were  responded  to. 

Hartley  (9)  obtained  a  very  definite  cortical  off- 
response  as  well  as  an  on-response  in  the  rabbit  when 
using  a  .slow  photic  pulse  rate  and  a  pulse-to-cycle- 
fraction  of  I  to  4.  The  results  are  pictured  in  figure  1 1 . 
In  it,  the  shape  and  temporal  characteristic  of  the 
wave  following  the  off-response  would  suggest  that  it 
is  the  usual  final  slow  component  of  a  typical  cortical 
response  or,  in  other  words,  an  alpha  wave.  If  this  be 
the  case,  then  it  is  suggested  that  the  off-response  may 
institute  an  alpha  series  in  the  same  sense  as  it  can  be 
said  that  a  brief  electric  stimulus  to  the  optic  nerve 
mav  do  so.  The  fact  that  an  off-response  is  at  all 
discernible  in  the  cortical  record  makes  the  supposi- 
tion that  the  off-response  plays  a  role  in  controlling 
critical  flicker  frequency  all  the  more  plausible. 

Brightness  Enhancement 

When  intermittent  photic  stimulation  is  used  at 
rates  below  those  producing  the  experience  of  steady 
light  (i.e.  at  subfusional  pulse  frequencies),  it  may 
become  more  effective  than  steady  stimulation  in  pro- 
ducing brightness.  This  increased  effectiveness  we  call 
'brightness  enhancement'  which  is  pictured  in  figure 
12.    With   intense    pulses,    effects   such   as   shown   in 


/ 

K 

0  MSEC 


200  400 

INTERVAL  BETWEEN  SHOCKS  TO   OPTIC  NERVE 


FIG.  13.  The  cycle  of  rcsponsixcnt'ss  ot  the  optic  cortex  of 
the  rabbit  as  determined  by  paired  stimulation.  [From  Hartley 
(4)-] 


curve  A  will  occur.  With  weaker  photic  radiation, 
results  shown  in  curve  B  will  occur.  While  it  is  to  be 
taken  for  granted  that  photochemical  processes  in 
sense  cells  play  their  usual  roles  in  determining  the 
magnitude  of  afferent  input  over  the  optic  nerve, 
they  do  not  account  for  the  nature  of  brightness  en- 
hancement. We  must  look  to  neurophysiological  proc- 
esses for  this. 

It  will  be  seen  from  the  diagram  in  figure  12  that 
the  effectiveness  of  intermittent  stimulation  increases 
as  pulse  rate  is  reduced,  and  that  under  some  condi- 
tions it  becomes  maximum  in  the  human  in  the  region 
of  10  pulses  per  sec.  This  region  is  the  peak  and  still 
slower  rates  result  in  reduced  effectivenesses.  One 
might  well  start  off  with  these  findings  and  make 
various  manipulations  of  pulse  rate,  pulse-to-cycle- 
fraction,  pulse  intensity,  etc.,  to  further  one's  under- 
standing of  brightness  enhancement  in  general.  The 
study  of  Ijrightness  enhancement  has  not  proceeded 
on  this  ba.sis.  The  work  that  has  provided  the  impetus 
for  brightness  enhancement  investigation  lay  in  the 
findings  of  neurophysiology  of  the  optic  pathway.  On 
this  account,  it  may  well  seem  much  clearer  to  the 
reader  were  we  to  describe  behavior  of  the  visual  re- 
sponse apparatus  before  continuing  to  deal  with 
brightness  enhancement. 

Bishop  and  Hartley,  in  their  study  of  cortical  re- 
sponse to  precise  stimulation  of  the  optic  nerve  in  the 
rabbit,  disclosed  a  number  of  temporal  and  intensive 
features  of  the  behavior  of  the  cortex.  Bishop  (18)  first 
demonstrated  the  rhythmicity  for  the  cortex  in  rela- 
tion to  peripheral  stimulation.  Stimuli  presented  to 
the  optic  nerve  at  intervals  without  regard  to  cortical 
events  produced  random-sized  responses.  He  showed 
that  stimuli  could  be  tuned  to  the  cortex,  so  that  all 
responses  would  be  essentially  the  same;  either  all 
small,  all  large  or  all  medium-sized,  depending  upon 
the  phase  to  which  the  input  was  tuned.  He  showed 
that  if  the  first  stimulus  in  a  train  was  maximal,  that 
it  would,  in  effect,  'drive'  the  cortex.  This  is  to  say, 
it  would  be  able  to  start  oflT  a  sequence  of  cortical 
consequences  having  the  properties  of  the  natural 
rhythm  but  shifted  somewhat  in  time  from  it.  Subse- 
quent closely-following  stimuli  would  obey  the  laws 
of  the  rhythmicity  but  according  to  the  shifted  timing. 
Hartley  (4)  mapped  the  nature  of  the  rh\thm  by 
using  paired  stimuli  systematically  varied  in  their 
separation.  He  found  that  the  size  of  a  second  maximal 
stimulus  to  the  optic  nerve  did  not  produce  a  cortical 
response  the  same  size  as  the  first  until  the  temporal 
interval  became  equal  to  the  cortical  period  found  by 
the  means  earlier  discovered.  The  findings  of  Bartley 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


733 


(4)  are  shown  in  figure  13,  in  which  it  is  indicated 
that  the  rhythm  could  be  followed  through  at  least 
about  two  cycles.  Since  maximal  stimuli  were  used,  it 
was  inferred  that  the  behavior  of  the  optic  nerve  as  a 
whole  represented  the  way  the  single  parallel  channels 
in  it  react.  This  deduction  rested  upon  the  idea  that 
all  parallel  channels  in  the  nerve  were  activated 
simultaneously.  This  cycle  represented  the  rhythm  of 
single  channels  while  being  the  rhythmicity  of  the 
system  as  a  whole  under  these  conditions. 

The  more  specific  portion  of  the  evoked  response  to 
brief  stimulation  is  followed  by  a  long-lasting  surface- 
negative  potential  (14).  It  is  during  this  time  that  a 
.second  brief  peripheral  impingement  evokes  either  no 
response  or  else  one  of  reduced  amplitude  ('25).  The 
repetition  of  the  cycle  implied  here  may  be  demon- 
strated at  the  frequency  of  the  spontaneous  alpha 
sequence.  Use  of  repetitive  stimulation  at  twice  alpha 
frequency  (4)  showed  that  an  original  inability  of  the 
system  to  respond  at  intervals  half  the  alpha  value 
slowly  changed  into  submaximal  response  following 
each  stimulus.  The  shift  was  as  if  the  channels  avail- 
able for  response  became  differently  distributed  in 
time,  so  that  finally  part  were  ready  to  respond  at  the 
presentation  of  one  stimulus  and  the  other  part  at  the 
presentation  of  the  succeeding  stimulus.  .Stimulation 
at  higher  multiples  of  the  alpha  rate  resulted  in  what 
appeared  to  be  a  further  redistribution,  such  that 
each  stimulus  was  responded  to  in  some  degree  but, 
of  course,  more  weakly  than  when  rates  were  slower 
(see  fig.  14). 

This  phenomenon  could  be  expected  to  have  a 
parallel  in  perceptual  response.  Whereas  a  certain 
rate  of  intermittent  stimulation  (c.f f)  is  required  to 
obliterate  flicker  fully  once  the  visual  system  is  ex- 
posed to  a  considerable  number  of  photic  pulses,  an 
even  slower  rate  may  fail  to  be  responded  to  as  indi- 
vidual pulses  at  the  very  onset  of  the  stimulus  train. 
Wilkinson  (68)  studied  this  problem  and  found  that 


the  rate  at  which  the  first  few  pulses  were  seen  as  indi- 
\idual  flashes  or  produced  flicker  was  much  below 
the  rate  at  which  the  pulses  could  still  produce  definite 
flicker  after  the  train  had  progressed  for  awhile.  The 
perceptual  responses  to  the  first  few  pulses  manifested 
some  of  essentially  the  same  irregularity  as  was  mani- 
fested in  the  cortical  response  (4)  under  the  same 
conditions. 

In  some  cases,  as  in  the  rabbit,  the  long-lasting 
surface-negative  wave  is  replaced  by  a  series  of 
briefer  waves  (25).  Something  like  this  was  observed 
in  the  cat  by  Bishop  &  O'Leary  (26).  Prior  to  the 
onset  of  the  depression  following  a  specific  cortical 
response,  it  was  found  that  a  short  period  of  facilita- 
tion to  a  second  stimulus  occurs  in  the  radiation  re- 
sponse (27).  For  example,  as  the  strength  of  an  initial 
stimulus  is  increa.sed,  the  response  of  the  radiation 
increases  more  rapidly  than  does  the  tract  response. 
With  a  second  stimulus,  it  and  the  first,  being  'maxi- 
mal' for  the  tract,  may  elicit  a  larger  radiation  re- 
sponse than  a  single  stimulus.  Even  below  maximal,  a 
second  stimulus  is  typically  more  effective  than  the 
first  when  falling  within  the  short  time  limits  implicit. 
This  indicates  that  spatial  and  temporal  summation 
are  operative  in  the  geniculate  even  with  "maximal 
stimuli.'  This,  although  indubitable,  is  inconsistent 
with  the  idea  of  a  one-to-one  fiber  channel  from  retina 
to  cortex  in  the  functional  sense.  It  is  consistent  with 
the  theory  of  partially  shifted  overlap  suggested  by 
Lorente  de  No  (53).  Similar  results  were  reported  by 
Marshall  &  Talbot  (56). 

Facilitation  may  occur  also  at  the  cortical  level.  The 
matter  is  far  more  complex,  however,  for  at  least  two 
reasons.  In  the  first  place,  the  t\pe  of  facilitation  just 
described  for  the  geniculate  occurs  at  each  cortical 
synapse.  That  is,  facilitation  builds  up  step  by  step 
at  each  synapse  in  the  sequence,  even  though  the 
facilitation  at  each  synapse  in  the  cortex  may  be  no 
greater  than  the  geniculate  synapse  facilitation.  The 


I       2       3       4      5      6       7       e       9      10      II       12     13      14     15     16      17     18 
REORGANIZATION    OF   RESPONSE    OF    CORTEX    TO     INTERMITTENT   STIMULATION 


FIG.  14.  The  response  of  the  optic  cortex  of  the  rabbit  to  rapidly  repeated  stimulation  of  the 
optic  nerve.  At  first  the  pulses  are  delivered  more  frequently  than  the  corte.\  is  able  to  respond. 
Later  to  this  same  rate,  the  several  channels  capable  of  being  activated  become  distributed  in 
time  in  such  a  way  that  no  single  channel  needs  to  respond  to  successive  pulses  for  there  to  be  a 
cortical  response.  [From  Hartley  (4).] 


734 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


second  reason  is  that  the  size  of  the  cortical  response 
varies  independently  from  the  amplitude  of  the  re- 
sponse of  the  geniculate,  owing  to  the  phase  of  the 
spontaneous  alpha  rhythm  that  is  involved  at  the 
time.  The  greater  responses  are  elicitable  during  the 
final  surface-positive  phase  of  the  cycle  (i8). 

The  depression  phase  following  activation  of  a 
pathway  may  be  a  more  general  thing  than  merely  a 
phenomenon  of  the  optic  pathway.  Such  a  phenom- 
enon was  dealt  with  by  Pitts  (6i)  in  respiratory  func- 
tion. Phasic  fluctuations  of  response  to  a  second  click 
stimulus  have  been  observed  in  the  auditory  tract  by 
Ro.senzweig  (63). 

As  an  extension  of  the  description  of  the  phasic 
nature  of  cortical  activity  both  spontaneous  and 
evoked,  and  thus  as  a  furtherance  of  the  understanding 
of  the  mechanisms  that  possibly  underlie  brightness 
enhancement,  we  shall  describe  the  changes  in  spon- 
taneous activity  of  the  cortex  following  a  single  optic 
nerve  stimulus. 

The  spontaneous  picture  in  the  rabbit  and  cat  dif- 
fers (21).  Whereas  in  the  rabbit  extended  periods  of 
alpha  wave  activity  are  t\pical,  in  the  cat  similarh 
dealt  with  one  may  find  short  trains  of  alpha  waves, 
but  more  often  this  is  replaced  by  a  continuous  rapid 
sequence  of  low-amplitude  waves  varying  in  fre- 
quency from  20  to  80  per  sec.  This  sort  of  sequence 
tends  to  appear  during  the  intervals  between  the 
"spindles'  of  alpha  wave  activity. 

If  stimulation  is  presented  during  this  fast-wave 
activity,  the  cortex  undergoes  a  characteristic  altera- 
tion. The  waves  just  mentioned  disappear  and  slowly 
come  back  over  a  period  of  from  100  to  200  msec.  At 
the  end  of  this  period,  the  amplitude  of  these  waves 
mav  be  far  above  normal  in  some  cases.  Sometimes 
the  waves  may  coalesce  into  longer  waves  as  if  two 
or  more  had  summed.  These  may  be  of  a  higher  am- 
plitude and  are  frequently  diphasic.  The  whole  se- 
quence, in  amplitude  variation  and  in  temporal 
features,  often  presents  the  over-all  envelope  of  the 
typical  alpha  wave. 

In  the  rabbit  also,  the  t\pical  alpha  wave  may  be 
replaced  at  times  by  three  or  more  peaks  with  the 
same  over-all  duration  as  the  alpha  wave  (26).  The 
differences  in  the  two  animals  presumably  consists  in 
a  smoother  summation  in  the  one  than  the  other, 
rather  than  in  the  presence  or  absence  of  the  alpha 
cycle.  The  depression  cycle  in  both  animals  seems  to 
be  a  recovery  from  the  peripheral  input  and  a  return 
to  spontaneous  activity. 

When  submaximal  rather  than  maximal  stimuli  are 
u.sed,  the  response  to  a  second  stimulus  of  a  pair  is 


le.ss  depressed.  The  spontaneous  activity  is  also  less 
depressed.  The  amplitude  of  the  specific  response 
during  the  depression  period  is  a  function  of  depres- 
sion in  both  the  geniculate  and  cortex,  but  the  re- 
covery in  the  cortex  seems  to  be  dependent  upon 
events  in  the  cortex  alone. 

The  basic  picture  of  how  the  overall  systems  must 
react  to  intermittent  inputs  was  summarized  by 
Bartley  (6)  in  what  he  called  the  "alternation  of  re- 
sponse' theory.  The  essentials  of  the  theory  are  as 
follows,  a)  There  is  a  fixed  number  of  parallel  channels 
in  the  optic  pathway  from  eye  to  brain.  6)  These 
channels  can  be  activated  simultaneously  or  they  can 
be  activated  according  to  the  various  temporal  distri- 
butions, f)  Certain  maximally  intense,  but  abrupt  and 
brief,  stimuli  may  activate  all  available  channels  while 
submaximal  stimuli  do  not.  d')  Any  given  single  chan- 
nel from  eye  to  cortex  cannot  be  reactivated  until  it 
has  recovered.  This  requires  about  0.2  sec.  for  the 
rabbit  and  o.  i  sec.  for  man.  i)  The  period  represented 
in  the  cycle  is  of  the  same  length  as  the  animal's  alpha 
rhythm.  In  fact,  it  is  the  alpha  rhythm,  as  was  indi- 
cated in  the  work  of  Bartley  (15).  /)  Brief  stimuli  de- 
livered at  the  alpha  rate  would  be  expected  to  produce 
maximal  brightness  effects,  g)  Not  only  must  the 
stimuli  be  intense  (maximal  or  in  the  upper  range  of 
intensity)  but  they  must  be  relatively  brief,  else  they 
would  involve  not  only  the  initial  activation  of  chan- 
nels but  also  the  reactivation  of  the  same  channels  or 
the  activation  of  still  others,  tending  to  spread  the 
over-all  activity  out  in  time  and  reduce  the  number  of 
channels  participating  in  the  responses  at  any  single 
instant.  /;)  Since  the  available  channels,  as  has  already 
been  said,  may  be  activated  not  only  in  unison  but 
also  in  various  temporal  distributions,  various  patterns 
of  the  latter  would  result  in  corresponding  levels  of 
sensory  brightness.  This  is  to  say  that  with  continuous 
stimulation  the  activity  of  the  available  channels  be- 
comes uniformly  distributed  throughout  the  cycle. 
There  would  be  as  many  channels  going  into  action 
at  all  instants  as  are  going  into  rest.  This  would  pro- 
vide for  continuous  uniform  visual  (sensory)  response 
to  continuous  illumination.  In  fact,  sensory  continuity 
and  steadiness  may  result  before  the  full  uniformity  of 
channel  activity  is  achieved. 

Bartley  (11,  12,  13,  16,  17)  and  colleagues  ha\e 
performed  a  number  of  sensory  experiments,  and  in 
all  cases  the  expectations  of  the  alternation  of  response 
theory  have  been  met. 

It  may  be  said,  then,  that  in  brightness  enhance- 
ment and  in  the  group  of  findings  in  regard  to  the 
way  in  which  the  optic  pathway  is  able  to  react  to 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


735 


timing  of  input,  we  ha\c  one  of  the  more  fully  docu- 
mented sets  of  the  relationship  ijciween  sensory  be- 
ha\  ior  (perception)  and  neurophysiology  of  the 
central  nervous  system. 

In  the  foregoing,  it  has  been  shown  that  stimulus 
conditions  for  obtaining  various  brightnesses  and 
those  for  obtaining  various  amplitudes  of  cortical 
response  are  the  same,  but  much  still  remains  to  be 
worked  out.  For  example,  the  comparison  seems  to 
pertain  to  the  quantitative  features  of  continuous 
over-all  brightness  and  the  amplitude  of  a  momentary 
feature  of  cortical  activity,  namely  the  specific  brief 
response.  As  yet,  there  is  no  characteristic  of  recorded 
electrical  response  of  the  cortex  to  indicate  the  level  of 
cortical  activity  in  response  to  a  continued  peripheral 
stimulus.  It  is  as  if  we  are  confined  to  dealing  exclu- 
sively with  momentary  and  brief  effects  in  the  central 
nervous  system.  They  can  be  dealt  with  because  they 
represent  an  observable  change  from  previous  or  from 
subsequent  activity  as  a  reference.  Continued  stimula- 
tion does  not  result  in  any  characteristics  of  prolonged 
cortical  activity  that  lend  themselves  to  useful  quanti- 
fication. One  approach  to  this  is,  of  course,  the  com- 
parison of  the  appearance  of  the  ongoing  cortical 
activity  during  stimulation  with  activity  in  the  absence 
of  an  intended  experimental  input.  This,  as  was 
implied,  does  not  give  anything  to  quantify  in  a 
direct  way.  The  chief  difference  between  cortical 
records  in  the  two  sets  of  conditions  seems  to  be  the 
disappearance  of  certain  forms  of  wave-like  activity 
in  the  'active'  record.  Various  studies  on  'blocking' 
the  alpha  rhythm  are  relevant  here  (64).  They  were 
also  relevant  in  the  earlier  section  on  brightness. 

Jasper  &  C^ruikshank  (49)  studied  the  electro- 
encephalograms of  human  subjects  exposed  to  a  cross- 
target  in  a  room  in  which  this  was  the  only  photic 
stimulation.  They  ascertained  the  change  in  the  cor- 
tical activity  picture  to  the  sudden  exposure  to  the 
target  and  the  subsequent  sequence  of  changes  that 
followed.  They  found  the  following:  a)  an  occasional 
and  \aried  short  detectal)lc  cortical  effect  arising  in  a 
few  milliseconds  easily  confused  midst  the  features  of 
the  alpha  rhythm;  A)  'blocking'  of  the  alpha  rhythm 
after  a  latency  of  160  to  520  msec. ;  c)  gradual  irregular 
reco\ery  of  the  alpha  rhythm  if  the  stimulus  continued 
for  more  than  3  to  5  sec. ;  dj  the  emergence  of  a  second 
dubious  positive  effect  that,  since  it  followed  the 
termination  of  exposure  to  the  target,  could  be  called 
an  'off'  effect;  i)  sometimes  a  second  'blocking,'  this 
time  of  the  recovered  rhythmic  activity,  following 
cessation  of  the  stimulus;/)  a  continued  depression  or 
'blocking'    effect    during;    the    existence    of   reported 


afterimages;  g)  a  partial  recovery  toward  the  usual 
amplitude  of  alpha  waves  between  successive  after- 
images; and  //)  a  final  total  recovery  of  the  normal 
alpha  activity  following  the  final  afterimage.  This 
recovery  typically  would  begin  as  a  train  of  small 
waves  of  higher  frequency  than  those  of  the  alpha 
rhythm.  The  amplitude  of  the  alpha  waves  might  even 
increase  for  a  while  before  the  full  prior  status  quo 
would  be  reached.  Here  we  have  a  set  of  results 
seeming  to  bear  upon  several  matters:  the  nature  of 
the  cortical  activity  during  continued  stimulation, 
and  the  fact  that  one  can  detect  cortical  response 
during  afterimages  as  being  different  than  when  they 
are  absent.  Others  have  been  interested  in  the  latency 
of  the  blocking  effect,  but  we  shall  forego  listing  the 
authors  or  the  exact  latencies  found. 

To  further  the  understanding  of  what  constitutes 
the  cortical  response  to  continued  stimulation,  certain 
reference  conditions  for  inactivity  will  have  to  be 
discovered.  From  these  comparisons  can  be  made. 
One  of  the  possible  leads  in  this  direction  may  be  the 
study  of  dendritic  behavior.  We  have  progressed  from 
the  exclusive  concern  with  and  ability  to  record 
spike-like,  momentary,  conducted  all-or-none  activity. 
The  activity  of  dendrites  seems  to  fall  into  the  category 
of  sustained  potentials  (36,  37).  While  sustained  states 
seem  to  be  'inactive'  ones,  since  we  cannot  detect 
them  as  ongoing  processes,  this  static  aspect  may  be 
only  the  over-all  aspect  of  the  whole  complex  of 
activities  that  is  in  operation,  thus  for  us  an  'illusion.' 

When  we  realize  that  what  is  happening  in  any 
mass  of  central  nervous  tissue  is  a  combination  of 
\arious  orders  of  process,  having  many  origins  and 
sustaining  conditions,  we  may  find  added  use,  in  our 
concept,  for  sustained  states.  They  may  be  the  sub- 
strata for  the  interplay  of  more  highly  particularized 
items  of  activity  that  occur  differently  and  play  dif- 
ferent roles  during  one  level  of  sustained  potential 
than  during  another.  It  thus  might  become  possible 
to  conceive  of  the  level  of  sustained  dendritic  potential 
in  crucial  areas  as  being  the  correlate  of  the  experi- 
ential or  motor  outcome  in  the  ultimate  response 
called  perceptual  behavior. 

In  essence,  the  idea  of  a  sustained  state,  varying  in 
significance  or  potency  according  to  its  level,  is 
nothing  new.  We  have  long  had  it  in  the  central 
excitatory  state  and  in  the  central  inhibitory  state  of 
Sherrington.  But,  to  understand  the  sustained  state  as 
being  inherent  in  a  neuron  rather  than  in  some  sort  of 
a  chemical  matrix  outside  it  is  very  different.  In 
dendritic  activity,  we  may  now  have  a  basis  for  sus- 
tained potentials  as  an  activity  of  neurons  themselves. 


736 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Bilateral  Fiinclions 

In  viewing  a  single  visual  target,  both  sides  of  the 
optic  pathway  are  generally  involved.  When  corre- 
sponding portions  of  the  two  retinas  receive  photic 
radiation  from  this  target,  the  results  are  as  follows. 
If  the  photic  flux  is  unequal  to  the  two  retinas,  the 
surface  seen  will,  of  course,  be  singular  but  will  not 
look  as  bright  as  though  viewed  by  the  eye  receiving 
the  greater  radiation.  That  is,  summation  does  not 
take  place.  This  is  Fechner's  paradox.  If,  instead, 
equal  radiation  is  received  by  both  eyes,  the  result  is 
summative.  The  brightness  is  greater  than  when  one 
or  the  other  eye  views  the  target  alone.  Fechner 
did  not  make  a  full  study  of  this  matter.  DeSilva  & 
Hartley  C39)>  snd  Fry  &  Hartley  (41)  manipulated 
stimulation  so  as  to  provide  curves  showing  this  func- 
tion under  a  wide  range  of  conditions  (see  fig.  15). 
Hartley  (lo)  later  studied  a  correlate  phenomenon  of 
Fechner's  paradox,  namely  the  way  the  pupil  behaves 
under  comparable  conditions  of  stimulation.  The 
same  pattern  of  quantitative  response  was  demon- 
strated. This  is  to  say  that  if  one  eye  alone  is  presented 
successive  increments  of  photic  radiation,  the  pupil 
constricts  step  by  step.  The  same  occurs  in  the  un- 
stimulated eye.  Then,  if  the  flux  to  the  first  eye  is  held 
at  the  final  high  level  and  step  by  step  the  flux  to  the 
second  eye  is  increased  from  zero  upward  to  the  level 
for  the  first  eye,  at  first,  the  paradoxical  reversal  of 
effect  occurs.  Instead  of  constricting  further,  the  two 
pupils  begin  to  dilate.  They  continue  to  do  so  as 
further  steps  of  increment  are  added  to  the  second  eye. 
Finally,  the  paradox  reaches  a  peak  and  further  incre- 
ments begin  to  cause  constriction.  When  finallv  both 
eyes  are  receiving  equal  amounts  of  radiation,  the  two 


pupils  have  constricted  more  than  when  the  final  level 
of  radiation  was  directed  to  the  one  eye  alone.  This  is, 
in  quantitative  pattern,  the  very  thing  that  happens 
in  perception. 

It  was  also  shown  that  the  essentials  of  the  paradox 
are  manifested  when  noncorresponding  points  of  the 
two  eyes  are  involved  and  finally  when  two  targets  of 
differing  inten.sity  are  imaged  on  two  parts  of  a 
single  retina. 

It  is  not  too  startling  to  find  the  parallel  between 
the  perceptual  and  the  motor  phenomena  when  once 
it  is  remembered  that  the  two  end  results  stem  from 
the  same  input,  namely  the  discharge  of  the  optic 
nerve.  The  same  pattern  of  input  must  go  to  the 
geniculate  and  to  the  superior  coUiculus.  In  our  illus- 
tration we  have  two  simple  aspects  of  the  respective 
categories  of  response.  Were  we  to  try  to  compare 
other  aspects  of  motion  and  perception  (experience), 
we  would  be  hard  put  to  find  modes  of  quantification. 
That  is  to  say,  it  would  be  very  difficult,  if  not  impos- 
sible, to  find  convincing  quantitative  parallels  in  limb 
movement  and  in  the  concurrent  perception  of  color 
or  position  of  a  seen  object.  Vet  there  must  be  rational 
(lawful)  relations  between  what  an  organism  sees  and 
where  it  reaches  to  grasp  the  object  seen. 

Binocular  relations  of  other  sorts  are  Ijroughi  out  in 
other  perceptual  phenomena.  If  two  fields  differing  in 
texture  or  color  as  seen  separately  are  presented,  one 
to  the  one  eye  and  the  other  to  the  second  eye  simul- 
taneously, the  result  may  not  be  a  fused  single  stable 
field  but  rather  a  single  field  that  alternates  in  texture 
or  color.  This  is  called  l^inocular  ri\alry.  Some  times 
this  ri\alry  in  lightness  is  replaced  by  a  curious  effect 
called  'luster.'  Further  analysis  shows  this  luster  to  be 
a  transparent  light  field  behind  which  is  a  dark  field. 


FIG.  15.  Binocular  summation  and 
subtraction  in  response  to  photic  stim- 
ulation. The  subtractive  effect  is  Fech- 
ner's paradox.  Top  line  indicates  the  seen 
brightness  when  both  eyes  are  presented 
equal  intensities.  Horizontal  line  ('one 
eye  alone')  indicates  the  seen  brightness 
when  only  one  eye  is  exposed  to  the 
target.  The  curve  shows  the  relative  seen 
Ijri^htncsscs  when  one  eye  is  exposed  to 
the  full  intensity  of  the  target  and  the 
second  eye  is  exposed  to  various  frac- 
tions of  full  intensity  shown  on  the  hor- 
izontal axis.  [From  Da  Silva  &  Bartlcy 
(39).] 


BOTH 

EYES 

u> 

> 

/ 

<n 

111 

ONE  EYE 

ALONE 

z  ^ 

111 

Sw 

> 

> 

V 

"* 

1- 

s. 

U— """^ 

0 

> 

111 

u. 

11. 

Ill 

111 

> 

p 

1. 

-1 

Ul 

K 

1/16    1/8  1/4  1/2  3/4  7/8    15/16 

RELATIVE    AMOUNT  OF    ILLUMINATION    PRESENTED    SECOND  EYE 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


737 


It  is  as  if  one  can  be  seen  through  the  other.  This  con- 
current existence  of  the  two  fields  is  best  brought  out 
when  one  uses  a  temporal  alternation  of  the  targets  for 
the  two  fields.  Thus  when  the  over-all  binocular  tar- 
get is  made  up  of  a  steady  annulus  surrounding  an 
intermittent  disk,  luster  eventuates  when  the  intensity 
of  the  annulus  is  one-half  that  of  the  positive  phase  of 
the  disk.  Whether  it  shows  up  or  whether  one  sees 
simply  light  and  dark  alternating  is  dependent  partly 
upon  the  rate  of  stimulus  intermittency.  As  one  slowly 
shifts  the  rate,  one  can  watch  the  phenomenon 
emerge. 

Rivalry  can  occur  even  when  the  target  \iewed  as 
first  described  constitutes  only  a  small  part  of  the 
visual  field.  Ri\alry  in  this  case  occurs  when  the  field 
surrounding  the  target  is  of  medium  brightness  and 
the  target  seen  via  one  eye  is  white  and  via  the  other  is 
black.  Apparently  the  neural  contour  processes  for 
the  two  targets  interact  in  some  way  that  involves  some 
sort  of  alternation,  thus  bringing  about  the  rivalry  (8). 

It  has  been  shown  by  Graham  (43)  that  the  abso- 
lute light  threshold  is  no  lower  for  the  two  eyes  than 
for  one.  This  finding,  though  it  might  be  unexpected, 
is  in  line  with  the  supposition  of  summation  at  a 
common  central  region,  as  found  by  Fry  &  Hartley 
(41).  The  latter  pointed  out  that  two  thresholds  must 
be  recognized :  a)  the  minimal  radiation  required  to 
activate  either  of  the  two  converging  pathways,  and 
A)  the  minimal  frequency  of  impulses  reaching  a  com- 
mon central  region  to  produce  postsynaptic  activity. 
The  perceptual  end  result  does  not  occur  unless  one 
or  the  other  of  the  two  pathways  delivers  the  threshold 
frequency.  If  in  either  one  of  the  two  the  minimum  is 
reached,  there  is  no  lowering  of  the  first  threshold  by- 
adding  a  stream  of  impulses  via  the  other  converging 
pathway. 

Brightness  Contrast 

Whereas  the  foregoing  illustration  (Fechner's  para- 
dox and  its  pupillary  analogue)  was  meant  to  demon- 
strate intensive  effects  based  upon  the  interaction  of 
the  two  sides  of  the  visual  apparatus,  it  also  exemplifies 
brightness  contrast  inasmuch  as  it  has  to  do  with 
adjacent  as  well  as  corresponding  areas  of  the  two 
retinas  and  with  adjacent  portions  of  a  single  retina. 
Brightness  contrast  pertains  to  adjacent  portions  of 
the  \isual  target,  but  to  explain  it  relevant  adjacent 
portions  of  the  visual  apparatus  must  be  dealt  with. 
It  would  seem  that  whatever  neural  mechanism  will 
account  for  Fechner's  paradox  will  go  a  long  way  in 
accounting  for  brightness  contrast. 


I  isual  Movement 

Brightness  contrast,  a  spatial  phenomenon,  is  a  con- 
figurational  one.  The  same  principle  would  seem  to 
apply  to  both  perceptual  and  neurophysiological 
phenomena  described  in  temporal  terms.  One  order 
of  temporal  phenomena  in  perception  is  the  experi- 
ence of  movement.  Very  often  the  crucial  neural  con- 
ditions underlying  movement  have  been  thought  to 
be  retinal  and  neuroretinal.  These  cannot  be  given 
space  here.  Be  it  sufficient  to  say  that  in  this  category 
lie  .some  of  the  conditions  for  apparent  visual  move- 
ment. Apparent  movement  is  defined  as  phenomenal 
(experienced)  movement  that  is  elicited  by  visual 
targets  that  do  not  undergo  displacement.  Real  move- 
ment is  the  movement  stemming  from  targets  that  do 
undergo  displacement. 

Despite  all  the  patterning  produced  in  the  retina, 
there  is  still  much  left  for  the  cortex  to  do.  The  cortex 
probably  plays  a  part  in  making  the  end  product 
resulting  from  optic  nerve  discharge  under  conditions 
of  target  displacement  often  very  similar  to  that  ob- 
tained with  target  fixity.  We  know  that  the  perceptual 
end  results,  in  some  cases,  are  indistinguishable. 

Since  in  beta  movement  (the  form  of  apparent 
movement  in  which  two  spatially  discrete  targets  are 
used)  there  is  a  temporal  gap  between  the  two  portions 
of  stimulation,  the  cortex  was  \ery  definitely  Ijrought 
in  by  early  workers  to  account  for  it.  Supposedly  some 
sort  of  spatial-temporal  coalescence  of  the  afferent 
discharge  into  the  cortex  was  finally  achieved  in  cor- 
tical activity  much  like  that  produced  by  the  periph- 
eral input  from  real-mo\'ement  targets.  An  early  form 
was  called  a  short-circuit  theory,  but  never  has  a 
theory  been  worked  out  to  the  point  of  being  con- 
N'incing.  It  would  seem  that  in  the  recent  establishment 
of  the  nature  of  dendrite  acti\ity,  we  have  an  essential 
tool  for  this  purpose.  Until  recently,  any  theorist 
wishing  to  account  for  certain  more  persistent  effects 
(those  lasting  up  to  seconds,  minutes  or  longer)  had 
to  rely  upon  purely  hypothetical  processes  such  as 
those  described  by  Kohler  (51)  or  upon  reverberative 
circuits.  Now  it  would  seem  that  with  the  demonstra- 
tion that  some  tissue  does  maintain  potential  and  not 
merely  conduct  potential  \ia  a  fleeting  impulse,  cer- 
tain more  slowly  changing  active  relations  between 
tissue  elements  or  central  nervous  regions  are  made 
more  concretely  thinkable. 

The  investigation  (4)  described  in  an  earlier  section 
on  cortical  localization  is  relevant  here.  Actually,  the 
stimulus  conditions  used  in  this  investigation  were 
the  very  ones  that  produce  apparent  visual  movement 


738 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


in  the  human  subject,  namely  the  exposure  of  two 
restricted  local  targets  separated  by  an  interspace  and 
presented  in  sequence.  To  go  with  this  target  arrange- 
ment from  conditions  that  are  not  productive  of 
apparent  movement  to  those  that  are  is  simply  to 
adjust  timing  and  spatial  separation  for  the  given 
intensity  used  (Korte's  laws).  In  this  investigation 
timing  and  spatial  separation  were  manipulated,  and 
it  was  demonstrated  that  cortical  responses  to  two 
separate  targets  could  be  recorded  at  two  separate 
cortical  locations  in  the  rabbit  and  that  various  inter- 
action effects  were  obtainable  when  space  separations 
were  reduced  and  when  the  delivery  of  the  stimuli 
was  made  close  together  in  time.  The  elements  of  a 
study  of  apparent  movement  were  demonstrated.  The 
in\estigation  did  not  go  far  enough  to  determine  the 
conditions  under  which  the  rabbit  responds  to  two 
stimuli  as  to  a  single  moving  target.  A  conditioning 
experiment  would  ha\e  Ijeen  necessary  for  this.  Thus, 
if  the  rabbit  could  be  'conditioned  to  apparent  move- 
ment,' the  cortical  experiments,  carried  further  than 
Bartley  (4)  was  able  to  do,  could  possibly  have  given 
a  picture  of  some  of  the  cortical  e\ents  involved  in 
seeing  movement. 

Color  Vision 

No  consideration  of  vision  should  bypass  what  is 
called  color  vision,  the  difTerential  response  to  the 
spectrum.  In  discussing  color  vision,  there  is  very 
often  some  confusion  as  to  what  is  really  meant,  owing 
to  the  fact  that  the  stimulus  differentiators  may  lie 
not  only  at  the  periphery  but  also  in  the  central  ner- 
vous system,  and  owing  to  the  possibility  of  setting  up 
diflferent  criteria  for  color  response.  There  are  actually 
si.x  items  to  keep  in  mind  and  make  clear  in  a  dis- 
cussion of  color  vision.  They  will  be  mentioned  here 
to  set  matters  straight,  a)  There  is  the  question  of  the 
existence  of  color  sense  cells  and  the  number  of  kinds 
of  such  cells  in  the  species  in  question,  i)  Often  this 
discus.sion  takes  the  form  of  whether  some  of  the  cells 
are  differentially  sensitive  to  the  spectrum  and  some 
not  sensitive  (cones  and  rods).  All  of  these  matters 
have  been  studied  on  an  anatomical  basis,  c)  There  is 
the  question  of  directly  or  indirectly  recording  elec- 
trical responses  to  answer  the  questions  in  a  and  h. 
d)  There  is  the  problem  of  obtaining  differential  con- 
ditioning of  overt  responses  to  the  spectrum  in  the 
species  in  question,  e)  In  human  subjects,  there  is  the 
study  of  color  experience.  /)  There  is  the  realization 
of  the  possibility  that  any  species  might  possess  a 
well-developed  spectral  analyzer  of  which  it  can  make 


little  or  no  use.  For  example,  the  eye  of  a  rabbit  or  a 
cat  or  a  monkey  may  be  quite  like  that  of  a  human, 
but  this  does  not  mean  that  in  any  or  all  of  these  cases 
there  is  the  same  color  experience.  In  fact,  we  know 
nothing  of  subhuman  experience  in  any  case. 

For  our  purposes  here,  we  want  to  know  the  role 
played  by  central  mechanisms  in  either  muscular  dif- 
ferential response  to  the  spectrum  or  in  the  production 
of  various  color  experiences.  Obviously,  even  though 
we  credit  the  retina  in  both  its  photochemical  and 
neural  mechanisms  as  being  a  keen  analyzer  and  thus 
providing  the  central  nervous  system  with  a  differen- 
tiated message,  the  central  apparatus  must  also,  in  a 
way,  be  an  analyzer,  else  it  cannot  make  differential 
use  of  the  message.  The  requirement  of  an  analyzer 
applies  both  to  the  center  and  the  periphery.  This  is 
made  apparent  to  those  possibly  more  difficult  to 
convince  by  the  fact  that  color  experiences  can  be 
predictably  elicited  by  nonspectral  stimuli.  Certain 
alternations  in  intensity  of  stimulation  as  produced  by 
a  rotating  disk  with  high-  and  low-reflecting  ('white' 
and  'black')  portions  are  sufficient  to  produce  color 
experience.  The  central  apparatus  responds  to  this 
nonspectral  presentation  in  the  same  fashion  as  to 
certain  spectral  presentations. 

The  foregoing  phenomena  taken  together,  or  many 
of  them  taken  alone,  lead  us  to  the  conclusion  that 
for  much  of  what  we  call  vision  we  must  include  the 
central  mechanisms  that  are  not  visual,  else  we  have 
nothing  that  can  be  called  vision.  It  is  customary  to 
call  the  surrounding  areas  association  areas,  but  we 
see  that  their  function  is  not  to  associate  rigid  units  of 
activity  each  of  which  plays  a  single  role  but  rather 
to  participate  in  the  overall  differentiation  of  activity 
we  call  response. 

Cortical  as  well  as  retinal  responses  to  spectral  stim- 
ulation (200  msec,  in  length)  have  been  recorded  by 
Lennox  &  Madsen  (52).  Simultaneous  records  from 
the  cortex  and  retina  were  compared  in  wa\e  form, 
amplitude  and  latency.  The  spectral  points  involved 
were  'blue'  (445  m^i);  'green'  (560  m^);  'yellow'  (575 
mti);  and  'red'  (620  mti). 

The  recordable  threshold  of  the  cortex  lay  about 
one  logarithm  below  the  retinal  threshold.  The  on- 
response  of  the  cortical  potential  consisted  in  a  di- 
phasic wave,  initially  surface-positive.  At  low  and 
moderate  intensities,  the  positive  response  was  double. 
At  high  intensities  the  initial  phases  contained  four 
or  five  spikelets. 

Increasing  stimulus  intensity  decreased  the  latency 
of  both   the  retinal   and   cortical   responses  and   in- 


CENTRAL    MECHANISMS    OF    VISION 


739 


creased  their  amplitudes.  The  spectral  composition  of 
the  stimulus  affected  shape,  amplitude  and  latency. 
The  two  components  of  the  positive  response  were 
most  marked  in  response  to  575  and  620  m|i  stimuli. 
The  amplitude  of  the  cortical  response  to  the  445  m/j 
stimulus  was  greater  than  to  the  560  m/i  stimulus 
when  in  the  retina  the  two  were  the  same.  The  la- 
tency of  the  cortical  response  to  the  445  m/j  stimulus 
was  longer  than  for  the  greater  wavelength  when 
under  the  same  circumstances  the  latencies  were  the 
same  at  the  retina.  The  final  conclusion  was  to  the 
effect  that  variation  in  the  cortical  responses  were  not 
solely  determined  at  the  periphery. 

The  same  two  authors,  Madsen  &  Lennox  (54), 
studied  cortical  response  to  spectral  stimuli  still  fur- 
ther. In  this  Study,  the  anterior,  mid  and  posterior 
optic  cortices  were  compared  by  means  of  simulta- 
neous recording.  The  double  positive  on-response 
mentioned  earlier  was  found  in  the  posterior  and  mid 
cortex.  The  response  from  the  anterior  cortex  was 
single.  The  maximum  of  the  wave  corresponded  to 
the  latency  of  the  second  peak  of  the  double  waves 
found  in  mid  cortex  within  a  value  of  from  2  to  5 
msec.  Latencies  on  the  anterior  cortex  were  signifi- 
cantly longer  than  in  posterior  cortex,  and  the  rate  of 
reduction  in  latency  with  increase  in  intensity  was 
more  rapid.  The  authors  attributed  the  difference  in 
latency  between  the  anterior  and  posterior  cortex  to 
the  absence  of  the  first  positive  peak  of  the  on-re- 
sponse in  the  anterior  cortex. 

The  latency  of  responses  to  the  445  m/i  stimulus 
was  shorter  at  the  anterior  position  and  that  for  red 
was  longer  than  at  the  posterior  position.  The  ampli- 
tude for  the  cortical  response  to  the  445  m/z  stimulus 
was  relatively  greater  at  the  anterior  than  at  the 
posterior  cortical  position. 


The  types  of  cortical  respon.ses  obtained  by  these 
authors  indicates  that  the  cortex  of  the  cat  does  re- 
spond differentially  to  spectral  stimulation.  In  direct 
contrast  to  this,  we  have  recent  evidence  for  thinking 
that  the  o\er-all  response  of  the  cat  (its  overt  beha- 
vior) does  not  utilize  the  differentials  of  cortical 
response  just  described.  Meyer  et  al.  (58)  were  unaijle 
to  condition  the  cat  differentially  to  the  photic  radia- 
tion passed  by  three  Wratten  filters  (23A,  'red'),  (47, 
'blue')  and  (61,  'green').  One  thousand  trials  were 
used  for  each  of  the  comparisons  of  filter  23A  with 
61,  and  47  with  61.  As  a  check,  a  pure  intensity 
comparison  was  used  and  conditioning  was  accom- 
plished in  200  trials.  This  led  the  authors  to  believe 
that  the  cat  does  not  possess  color  vision. 

The  fact  that  differential  cortical  responses  to  spec- 
tral stimulation  can  be  detected  and  yet  the  same  spe- 
cies cannot  be  taught  to  respond  differentially  in  its 
overt  behavior  is  a  concrete  example  of  the  principle 
which  we  stated  earlier  in  this  section.  It  is  an  exam- 
ple of  why  we  need  to  be  quite  definitive  in  what  we 
mean  when  we  use  the  term  color  vision. 

To  clarify  matters,  one  had  better  never  speak  of 
color  vision  in  subhuman  species.  If  the  animals  in 
question  can  be  trained  to  respond  differentially  to 
various  parts  of  the  spectrum,  we  can  call  the  beha- 
vior overt  spectral  vision.  Color  vision  is  a  term  that 
should  be  reserved  for  the  description  of  human  ex- 
perience. If  neurophysiological  experiments  indicate 
differential  response  to  the  spectrum  by  any  or  all 
sense  cells,  then  it  should  simply  be  called  spectral 
response.  Vision  is  not  a  term  to  apply  to  sense  cell 
behavior.  Thus  we  have  three  categories  of  behavior 
to  talk  about,  spectral  response,  spectral  vision  and 
color  vision,  and  it  is  in  the  interests  of  clarity  that  we 
use  three  terms. 


REFERENCES 

1.  Adri.'\n,  E.  C.  and  G.  Moruzzi.  J.  Fhisinl.  97:  153,   1939. 

2.  B.\RTLEv,  S.  H.  Am.  J.  Phsiiil.   108:  387,   1934. 

3.  Hartley,  S.  H.  Am.  J.  Physinl.    iio:  666,    1935. 

4.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  8:  41,   1936. 

5.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  Am.  J.  Physiol.    117;  338,   1936. 

6.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  Proc.  Soc.  Exper.  Biol.  &  Med.  38:  535,  1938. 

7.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  Psychol.  Rev.  46:  337,   1939. 

8.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  25:  462,   1939. 

9.  Bartlev,  S.  H.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  27;  624,   1940. 

10.  Hartley,  S.  H.  J.  Exper.  Psychol.  32:   110,   1943. 

11.  Hartley,  S.  H.  J.   Psychol.   32:  47,    1951. 

12.  Hartley,  S.  H.  J.  Psychol.  32:  217,  1951. 

13.  Hartley,  S.  H.  J.  Psychol.  34:   165,   1952. 

14.  Hartley,  S.  H.   a.nd  G.   H.   Bishop.   .Am.  J.  Physiol.    103: 
159.  '933- 


15.  Bartlev,  S.   H.,   J.  0'Le.'\rv  and  G.   H.   Bishop.  Am.  J. 
Physiol.   120:604,  '937- 

16.  Hartley,  S.  H.,  G.  Paczewitz  and  E.  Valsl  J.  Psychol. 
In  press. 

17.  Hartley,  S.  H.  and  F.  R.  Wilkinson.  J.  Psychol.  33:  301, 

195^- 

18.  Bishop,  G.  H.  .Am.  J.  Physiol.    103:  213,   1933. 

19.  Bishop,   G.   H.   .\nd   M.   Clare.  J.  .\europhyswl.    14:   497, 

1951- 

20.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  M.  H.  Cl.\re.  J.  .\europhysiol.  15:  201, 

i95'^- 

21.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  M.  H.  Clare.  Eleclroencephalog.  &  Clin. 
.Kemophysiol.  4:  321,  1952. 

22.  Bishop,  G.  H.   and  M.   H.   Clare.  J.  .Veurophysiol.    16:   1, 
1953- 


740 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  --^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


■23.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  M.  H.  Clare.  J.  Meurophysiol.  16:  490, 

1953- 

24.  Bishop,  G.   H.   and  M.   H.   Clare.  J.  Comp.  Neurol.    103: 

269,  1955- 

25.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  J.  O'Learv.  Am.  J.  Physiol.   117:  292, 
1936. 

26.  Bishop,   G.   H.   and  J.   O'Learv.    7-   .^europhysiol.    i:   391. 

1938- 

27.  Bishop,   G.   H.   and  J.   O'Le.arv.  J.  Aeurophysiol.   3:   308, 
1940. 

28.  Bishop,  G.  H.  and  J.  0'Le.\rv.  J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol. 

19:  315.  194^- 

29.  Bishop,   P.  O.,  D.   Jeremy  and   J.   B.   McLeod.  J.  .Xeuro- 
physiol.  16:  437,  1953. 

30.  Bishop,  P.  O.  and  J.  B.  McLeod.  J.  .\euiophysiol.  17:  387, 

>954- 

31.  Bremer,   F.   Electioencephalo«.  &  Clm.  Xewophysiol.    i:    177, 

'949- 

32.  Chang,   H.  T.  J.  .\europhysiul.    15:  5,    1952. 

33.  Chanc,   H.   T.   and   B.    Kaada.  J.   Neurophyswl.    13:   305, 

1950- 

34.  Clare,  M.  H.  .\nd  G.  H.  Bishop.  Eleclmencephaloa.  Si  Clin. 

Neurophyswl.  4:  311,  1952. 

35.  Cl.are,  M.  H.  and  G.  H.  Bishop.  J.  .\europh\swl.  17:  271, 

1954 

36.  Cl.^re,  M.  H.  .-^nd  G.  H.  Bishop.  Elee/roencephalog.  &  Clin. 
Neurophyswl.  7:85,  1955. 

37.  Clare,  M.   H.   and  G.   H.   Bishop.  Am.  J.   Psychiat.    1 1  i  : 
818,  1955- 

38.  Dempsey,   E.   W.,   R.   S.   Morison   and  B.   R.    Morison. 
Am.  J.  Physiol.   131:  718,   1941. 

39.  DeSilva,  H.  R.  and  S.  H.  Barti.ey.  But.  J.  Psychol.  20: 
241,  1930. 

40.  Eccles,   J.  C.  Elect} oencephalog.  ^  Clin,  .\europhysiol.  3:  449, 

1951- 

41.  Fry,  G.  a.   and  S.   H.   Bartley.  .4m.  J.   Ophlh.    16;  687, 

'933- 

42.  Gastaut,  H.,  Y.  a.  Roger,  J.  Coriol  .\nd  R.  N.^guet. 
Electroencephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol .  3:  401,   1951. 

43.  Graham,   C.   H.  J.  genet.  Psychol.   3:  492,    1930. 

44.  Halstead,  W.  C,  G.  W.  Knox  and  .\.  E.  Walker.  J. 
Neurophysiol.  5:  349,  1942. 


45- 
46. 

47- 
48. 

49- 

5°- 

5'- 


54- 

55- 
56. 

57- 

58. 

59- 
60. 
61. 
62. 

63- 
64. 

65- 
66. 
67. 
68. 


Halstead,  W.  C.,  G.  W.  K.nox,  J.  I.  VVoolf  and  A.  E. 

Walker.  J.  Neurophysiol.   5:  483,    1942. 

Jasper,  H.  H.  XI  Congr.  Internal.  Psychol.^  Rapp.  et  Communic. 

226,  1937. 

J.ASPER,  H.  H.  Electroencephalog.  <i  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  i  :  405, 

'949- 

Jasper,  H.  H.,  C.  Ajmone-Mars.\n  and  J.  Stole.  A.M  .{ 

Arch,  .\eiirol.  &  Psychiat.   67:    155,    1952. 

Jasper,  H.   H.   and  R.   M.  Cruikshank.  J.  genet.  Psychol. 

17:  ^9.  ■937- 

Kluver,   H.    Buil.   .S'ymp.    7:   253,    1942. 

Kohler,  W.   Dynamics  in  Psychology.  New  York  :  Li\  eri^ht. 

1940. 

Lennox,  M.  .\.  and  .\.  Madsen.  J.  Neurophyswl.   18:  413, 

■955- 

LoRENTE  DE  N6,   R.  J.  Psychol.  u.  Neurol.  46:   113,   1934. 

Madsen,  A.  .and  M.  .\.  Lennox,  j.  .Veurophysiol.  18:  574, 

'955- 

M.ARSH.iiLL,   W.    H.  J.   .\euruphysiol.    12:   277,    1949. 

Marshall,  W.  H.  and  S.  .A.  Talbot.  Am.  j.  Physiol.  129: 

417,  1940. 

Marshall,   W.   H.,   S.   .\.   T.\lbot  and   H.   W.   .\des.  J. 

.\europhysiol.  6;  r,  1942. 

Meyer,  D.  R.,  R.  C.  Miles  and  P.  Ratoosh.  J.  .\euro- 

physiol.   17:  289,  1954. 

O'Leary,  j.  L.  j.  Comp.  Neurol.  62:   117,    1935. 

PiERON,   H.   Compt.  rend.  Soc.  de  biol.    11 1:  380,    1932. 

Pitts,  R.  F.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  439,   1943. 

RiocH,   D.   McK.  j.  Comp.  Neurol.   49:    i,    1929. 

RosENZWEiG,  M.   R.  .4m.  J.  Physiol.    163;  746,    1950. 

St-AMM,    j.   S.   Electroencephalog.  &   Clin.  Neurophysiol.   4;  61, 

■95^- 

,St.\rzl,  T.   E.   and  H.   W,   M.\goun.  J.  Neurophyswl.    14: 

133.  '951- 

Thomas,  L.  B.  and  F.  L.  Jenkner.   Tr.  Am.  .Veurol.  A.  77: 

47.  '95-!- 

Walker,   .A.    E.,  J.   T.    Woolf,   W.    C.    Halstead   and 

T.  J.  Case.  J.  .\'europkysiol.  6:  253,   1943. 

Wilkinson,   F.   R.    The  Organization  of  the   Visual  Response 

(Thesis).   East   Lansing,   Michigan:   Michiejan   State   L'ni- 

versity,  1955. 


CHAPTER    XXXI 


Central  control  of  receptors 
and  sensory  transmission  systems 


ROBERT    B.   LIVINGSTON 


National  Institute  oj  Menial  Health  and  National  Institute  of  Neurological 
Diseases  and  Blindness,  National  Institutes  of  Health,  Bethesda,  Maryland 


CHAPTER     CONTENTS 

Control  of  Receptor  Acti\  ity 

Sympathetic  Influence  on  Touch  Receptors 

Efferent  Control  of  Invertebrate  Stretch  Receptors 

Efferent  Control  of  Mammalian  Stretch  Receptors 

Remote  Central  Control  of  Stretch  Receptors 
Control  of  Activity  in  Special  Sense  Afferents 

Auditory  Nerve  Activity 

Optic  Nerve  Activity 

Olfactory  Bulb  Activity 
Control  of  Central  Sensory  Relays 

Spinal  Ascending  Relays 

Dorsal  Column  and  Other  Bulbar  Relays 

Thalamic  Relays 
Cephalic  Interaction  Systems 

Corticipetal  Projection  Systems 

Cortical  Interaction  Systems 

Corticifugal  Influences  on  Brain-Stem  Mechanisms 

Organization  of  Centrifugal  Sensory  Control  Mechanisms 

Transactional    Mechanisms    Relating     to    Sensory    Control 
Systems 
Sensory  Attention,  Habituation  and  Conditioning 

Auditory  Habituation 

Auditory  Conditioning 

Shifts  of  Attention 

Visual  Responses 

Beha\ior  and  Neurophysiology 
Interpretations 
Summary 


IT  IS  A  VER"!'  OLD  NOTION,  which  needs  often  to  be 
repeated,  that  our  sensory  pathways  are  subject  to 
error  and  hence  may  yield  distorted  sensations.  This 
idea   was   succinctly   stated    three   centuries   ago   by 


Descartes,'  in  point  of  fact,  these  essentially  neuro- 
physiological  considerations  provided  the  cornerstone 
of  his  philosophy  of  universal  doubt.  Nonetheless, 
little  attention  has  been  given  to  the  possibility  that 
the  central  nervous  system  may  itself  be  able  to 
exercise  some  measure  of  direct  control  over  the 
traffic  of  nerve  impulses  ascending  sensory  pathways. 

Recent  experimental  evidence  indicates  that  such 
central  influences  do  exist  and  can  modify  sensory 
input  patterns  all  the  way  from  receptors  to  whate\er 
end  point  is  chosen — from  peripheral  sense  organs  to 
at  least  the  sensory  cortex.  Much  additional  study 
needs  to  be  given  to  particular  features  of  this  mech- 
anism, but  already  the  implications  are  far-reaching. 

Sensory  impulses  can  apparently  be  interfered  with 
at  their  point  of  origin  and  at  synaptic  junctions  as  a 
result  of  activity  taking  place  in  certain  remote  parts 

'  "I  have  learned  from  some  persons  whose  arms  or  legs  have 
been  cut  off,  that  they  sometimes  seemed  to  feel  pain  in  the  part 
which  had  been  amputated,  which  made  me  think  I  could  not 
be  quite  confident  that  it  was  a  certain  member  which  pained 
me,  even  although  I  felt  pain  in  it.  .  .  .  In  the  same  way,  when 
I  feel  pain  in  my  foot,  my  knowledge  of  physics  teaches  me  that 
this  sensation  is  communicated  by  means  of  ner\es  dispersed 
through  the  foot,  which  being  extended  like  cords  from  there 
to  the  brain,  when  they  are  affected  in  the  foot,  at  the  same  time 
affect  the  inmost  portion  of  the  brain  which  is  their  extremity 
and  place  of  origin,  and  there  excite  a  sensation  of  pain  repre- 
sented as  existing  in  the  foot.  ...  If  there  is  any  cause  which 
excites,  not  in  the  foot  but  in  some  part  of  the  ner\es  which  are 
extended  between  the  foot  and  the  brain,  or  even  in  the  brain 
itself,  the  same  action  which  usually  is  produced  when  the  foot 
is  detrimentally  affected,  pain  will  be  experienced  as  though  it 
were  in  the  foot."     Rene  Descartes,  Discourse  on  Method,  1637. 


741 


742 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


of  the  nervous  system.  This  interference  inxoKes  an 
active  process  that  is  usually  inhibitory.  In  the  waking 
state,  the  sensory  pathways  seem  ordinarily  to  be 
under  a  tonic  inhibitory  influence;  evidenth  a  good 
deal  of  the  potential  content  of  sensory  experience  is 
being  continuously  reduced  or  eliminated  within  the 
initial  stages  of  sen.sory  integration.  Inasmuch  as 
activity  along  sensory  pathways  appears  to  be  modifi- 
able to  some  extent  according  to  an  animal's  en\iron- 
mental  experience  and  according  to  its  overtly  ex- 
pressed direction  of  attention,  the  interference  with 
sensory  transmission  appears  to  be  regulatory  and  to 
constitute  a  goal-seeking  physiological  mechanism. 

These  findings  call  for  some  adjustment  of  current 
physiological,  psychological  and  philosophical  con- 
cepts relating  to  perception.  Most  such  concepts  have 
been  based  upon  a)  physiological  findings  deri\ed 
from  an  examination  of  anesthetized  animals,  findings 
which  usually  reveal  the  activities  of  only  a  few  parts 
of  the  nervous  system  at  a  time,  and  A)  behavioral 
evidence  obtained  with  waking  unanesthetized  ani- 
mals in  which  the  nervous  system  is  treated  as  a  whole. 
Some  degree  of  closure  between  these  two  experi- 
mental realms  of  .science  was  apparently  achieved  30 
years  ago.  Adrian  and  other  physiologists  discovered 
that  the  strength  of  a  stimulus  necessary  to  elicit 
action  currents  in  peripheral  sensory  nerves  of  anes- 
thetized animals  was  approximately  equal  to  that 
found  by  psychologists  for  threshold  perception  in 
attentive  human  subjects  (4,  chapter  VI}.  Compa- 
rable stimuli,  again  in  anesthetized  animals,  were  then 
found  to  yield  evoked  cortical  responses  that  were 
localized  to  certain  'sen.sory  receiving'  areas  of  the 
cortical  mantle  (59).  Detailed  analysis  in  anesthetized 
animals  of  activity  taking  place  within  various  relay 
stations  between  the  peripheral  nerves  and  the  cortex 
re\-ealecl  that  the  spinal  (80),  brain-stem  (60)  and 
thalamic  synaptic  relays  (69}  were  quite  reliable  in 
their  transmission  of  evoked  signals. 

Naturally  such  findings  led  to  an  interpretation 
that  the  sen.sory  nerves  and  the  central  ascending 
paths  reliably  convey  to  the  cortex  whatever  messages 
are  generated  by  the  sensory  end  organs.  It  was  argued 
that  only  when  the  impulses  reach  the  cortex  are  they 
then  accessible  to  such  p.sychological  factors  as  habit- 
uation, focus  of  attention,  suggestion,  etc.,  long  known 
to  intervene  in  sen.sory  perception.  The  cortex  was 
believed  to  be  only  the  first  stage  in  the  integration 
of  sensation  from  sense  data  (7,  pp.  39,  40,  62).  This 
view  fitted  well  with  the  traditional  conception  of 
hierarchical  supremacy  of  the  cortex — notions  de- 
ri\ecl  partly  from  the  recognition  of  its  topmost  loca- 


tion, enormous  areal  extent,  anatomical  complexitv, 
phylogenetic  recency,  etc.,  and  partly  from  the  mo- 
mentum of  theoretical  conceptions  of  Pa\lo\-  and 
others  who  assigned  most  psychological  functions  to 
the  cortex  (64). 

^  et  for  more  than  50  years  anatomists  have  recog- 
nized that  certain  nervous  pathways  enter  sensory 
nuclei  and  relay  stations  from  above,  and  that  nearly 
all  .sensory  systems  have  eff"erent  fibers  passing  from 
the  neuraxis  to  receptor  organs.  When  the  indi\idual 
anatomical  features  of  these  centrifugal  projections 
are  grouped  together,  they  appear  to  constitute  a 
series  of  descending  neuronal  cascades  which  con- 
ceivably might  have  an  influence  upon  ascending 
sensorN  impul.ses.  These  descending  and  efl"erent  sen- 
sory projections  ha\e  usually  been  considered  piece- 
meal and  few  conceptual  generalizations  are  available. 
Perhaps  the  most  prophetic  of  these  appears  in  an 
interpretive  discussion  of  neuropathology  by  Brouwer 
in  1933:  ".  .  .  We  accept  that  there  is  also  a  cen- 
trifugal side  in  the  process  of  sensation,  of  \ision,  of 
hearing,  and  so  on.  I  believe  that  a  further  anahsis 
of  these  descending  tracts  to  pure  sensory  centers  will 
also  help  physiologists  and  psychologists  to  under- 
stand some  of  their  experiences"  (10,  p.  627). 


CONTROL  OF  RECEPTOR  .ACTIVITY' 

Sympalhetif  Iiifluemt'  nii   Touch  Receptors 

Single  touch  receptor  activity  in  isolated  skin  areas 
of  the  frog  can  be  facilitated  in  stimulation  of  the 
sympathetic  nerve  supply  to  that  region  (56).  Activity 
in  these  receptors  can  also  be  facilitated  by  the  local 
application  of  epinephrine  or  norepinephrine,  or  by 
introducing  these  hormones  into  the  circulation. 
Thus,  individual  receptors  are  evidently  subject  to 
generalized  as  well  as  local  sympathetic  influences. 
Sympathetic  ner\e  influences  have  alreadv  been 
shown  to  be  facilitatory  to  transmission  across  the 
neuromuscular  junction  (see  56  for  references);  their 
effects  on  touch  receptor  acti\it\-  therefore  appear  to 
be  parallel  and  to  place  the  peripheral  sensory  as  well 
as  peripheral  motor  portions  of  the  reflex  arc  under 
some  degree  of  central  control.  By  \irtue  of  these  in- 
fluences, the  reflex  arcs  relating  to  touch  should  no 
longer  be  considered  such  simple  units  of  neuro- 
physiological  and  behasioral  systems.  Since  appar- 
ently all  sensory  receptors  receive  sympathetic  fibers, 
it  is  perhaps  not  too  extra\agant  a  generalization  to 
suppo.se  that  all  of  them  may  be  found  susceptible  to 
this  kind  of  central  interference. 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


743 


Efferenl  Cunlinl  nf  Irivetiehrale  Stretch  Rcajitors 

Another  central  control  mechanism  relating  to 
peripheral  afferent  nerve  discharge  has  been  dem- 
onstrated in  crustacean  stretch  receptors  by  Kuffler  & 
E\zaguirre  (49)-  They  have  shown  that  the  stretch- 
sensitive  muscle  afferent  in  the  crayfish  tail  is  itself 
innervated  by  an  efferent  inhibitory  ner\e  fiber  which 
can  diminish  or  arrest  the  activity  of  the  afferent  fiber. 
The  afferent  ner\e  discharge  that  is  ordinarily  elicited 
by  a  given  muscle  stretch  can  be  decreased  or  oblit- 
erated depending  on  the  rate  and  number  of  impulses 
delivered  to  the  inhibitory  fiber.  Presumabh'  this  sort 
of  control  can  be  effected  by  central  ganglia  in  the 
intact  cravfish. 


Efferenl  Crjiitrol  oj  Mainmalnin  Stretch  Receptors 

The  rate  of  discharge  of  the  large  mammalian 
muscle-spindle  afferent  apparentK'  depends  upon  the 
degree  of  tension  developed  by  a  small  intrafusal 
mu.sclc  fiber  contained  within  the  spindle.  This  intra- 
fusal fiber  can  be  passively  stretched  or  relaxed  along 
with  lengthening  or  shortening  of  the  surrounding 
skeletal  muscle.  In  addition,  it  has  its  own  motor  con- 
trol by  way  of  the  small  ventral  root  gamma  efferents 
(40,  50-52).  Thus,  the  discharge  of  spindle  afferents, 
which  play  such  an  important  role  in  proprioception, 
is  determined  both  by  the  state  of  the  skeletal  muscle 
and  by  the  rate  of  discharge  of  the  gamma  efferents. 

The  gamma  efferents  enable  the  spindle  afferents 
to  have  a  full  range  of  discharge  rates  for  any  given 
muscle  length,  the  end  result  being  a  better  accom- 
modation of  different  loads  and  rates  of  movements. 
It  can  readily  be  appreciated  that  this  peripheral 
feedback  or  loop-gain  system  provides  an  exceedingly 
important  measure  of  central  control  over  sensory 
input. 


Remote  Central  Control  of  Stretch  Receptors 

Granit  &  Kaada  (30)  discovered  that  the  gamma 
efferents  controlling  muscle-spindle  afferents  are  in 
turn  regulated  by  a  number  of  remote  central  struc- 
tures. As  shown  in  figure  i,  muscle-spindle  discharges 
are  readily  accelerated  by  stimulating  the  mesence- 
phalic and  diencephalic  reticular  formation — the 
brain-stem  facilitatory  region  of  Magoun  (28,  30). 
When  these  structures  are  activated,  a  mu.scle-spindle 
afferent  will  continue  to  show  facilitation  for  up  to 
half  a  minute  or  more  following  discontinuation  of 
the  brain-stem  excitation.   Similar  but  less  uniform 


13 


16 


FIG  I.  Effect  of  brain-stem  reticular  (midbrain  tegmentum) 
stimulation  on  a  gastrocnemius  muscle  spindle  afferent  dis- 
charge. Above:  Contraction  of  134  gm  at  low  myograph  sensi- 
tivity to  demonstrate  silent  period  of  the  large  muscle  spindle 
afferent  unit.  Initial  tension  throughout,  52  gm.  Light  Dial- 
chloralose  anesthesia.  /  to  ./:  Control  before  reticular  stimula- 
tion. 5  to  //:  During  stimulation.  12  to  31:  After  stimulation. 
Consecutive  sweeps  at  2  sec.  intervals.  Myograph  (M)  alongside 
film.  Distance  M-B  (base  line)  corresponds  to  10  gm.  Note  that 
stimulation  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation,  without 
altering  the  muscle  tension,  accelerates  the  spindle's  rate  of 
firing  and  that  this  effect  persists  more  than  half  a  minute. 
[From  Granit  &  Kaada  (30).] 


744 


HANDBOOK    (JF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


effects  are  elicited  by  stimulating  the  motor  cortex, 
the  anterior  lobe  of  the  cerebellum,  the  habenular 
complex  and  the  head  of  the  caudate  nucleus.  Inhibi- 
tion of  spindle  activity  is  readily  elicited  by  stimula- 
tion of  the  medial  part  of  the  bulbar  reticular  forma- 
tion— the  brain-stem  inhibitory  region  of  Magoun 
— and  by  excitation  of  the  anterior  lobe  of  the  cere- 
bellum (17;  28,  p.  103;  30).  Eldred  has  painstakingly 
extended  the  exploration  and  anahsis  of  these  remote 
central  spindle  afferent  control  mechanisms  (16). 

Granit  &  Kaada  showed  that  gamma  efferent 
activity  is  facilitated  by  reticular  stimulation  at 
strengths  considerably  below  those  which  will  elicit  a 
discharge  of  the  large  skeletal-muscle  (alpha)  moto- 
neurons. Hence,  motor  facilitation  by  brain-stem 
mechanisms  appears  to  take  place  first  through  an 
activation  of  the  gamma  efferents  controlling  sensory 
input  from  the  muscle  spindles,  and  then  bv  both  the 
direct  descending  influences  which  act  upon  the  large 
motoneurons  and  the  continuing  indirect  influence  of 
brain-stem  control  over  muscle-spindle  afferent  dis- 
charges which  act  back  upon  the  same  motor  units. 
As  in  other  sensory  control  systems,  the  gamma  effer- 
ents appear  to  be  normally  under  a  tonic  inhibitory 
influence  from  above. 

In  each  example,  the  frog  tactile  receptor,  the 
muscle  stretch  receptor  in  Crustacea  and  the  mam- 
malian muscle-spindle  afferent,  there  is  evidence  for 
efferent  neuronal  systems  which  exercise  an  important 
controlling  effect  upon  the  initiation  of  afferent  nerve 
impulses.  In  the  case  of  the  muscle  spindle,  at  least, 
the  efferent  fibers  are  in  turn  under  the  control  of 
certain  remote  central  mechanisms.  The  principle  of 
central  control  of  afferent  activity  is  equally  applicable 
to  the  special  senses. 


CONTROL  OF  ACTIVIT\-  IN  SPECIAL  SENSE  AFFERENTS 

Auditory  Nerve  Activity 

For  many  years  a  compact  bLuidlc  of  libers  traveling 
with  the  eighth  cranial  nerve  pair  was  considered  to 
be  afferent  (65,  vol.  I,  figs.  319,  324).  In  a  series  of 
critical  anatomical  studies,  Rasniussen  proved  that 
these  are  really  efferent  fibers.  They  arise  in  the 
vicinity  of  the  superior  olive  and  terminate  within  the 
contralateral  cochlea  (67,  68).  Rasmussen's  efferent 
fibers  appear  to  make  contact  with  the  afferent  audi- 
tory fibers  as  these  pass  from  the  hair  cells  to  the 
spiral  ganglion.  Some  of  the  efferents  may  pass 
directly  to  the  inner  hair  cells  but  this  point  is  un- 


LEFT  0-C 
CUT 


/t^tN""*^ 


LEFT  STAPEDIUS 
CUT 


FIG.  2.  Suppression  of  auditory  nerve  response  by  olivococh- 
lear and  stapedius  mechanisms.  A.  Control  auditory  nerve 
responses  to  click  applied  to  each  ear,  right  recording  above 
left.  B.  Suppression  of  both  left  and  right  responses  with  shocks 
at  100  per  sec.  delivered  to  the  decussation  of  the  olivocochlear 
bundle  in  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle.  This  high  frequency 
of  stimulation  tetanizes  the  stapedius  muscle  so  as  to  eliminate 
interference  from  that  source  (see  E  below).  C.  Following 
transection  of  the  left  olivocochlear  bundle,  the  suppression 
shown  in  B  occurs  only  on  the  right.  D.  .Another  control  re- 
sponse showing  that  lesion  made  between  B  and  C  has  not 
interfered  with  auditory  nerve  response  from  either  ear.  E. 
Single  shocks  to  same  medullary  location  1 3  msec,  prior  to  test 
clicks  suppress  the  eighth  nerve  responses  from  either  ear 
(stapedius  effect).  F.  Following  cutting  of  the  tendon  of  left 
stapedius  muscle  the  suppression  shown  in  E  is  seen  only  on 
the  right.  [From  Galambos  (26).] 


settled.  Galambos  has  recently  shown,  as  illustrated 
in  figure  2,  that  stimulation  of  the  medulla  in  the 
region  of  the  superior  olive,  and  along  the  course  of 
the  olivocochlear  bundle,  will  cause  a  suppression  of 
auditory  nerve  responses  elicited  by  standard  click 
stimulation  (26).  Such  suppression  does  not  occur 
following  division  of  the  olivocochlear  bundle  at  a 
point    peripheral    to    the   locus   of  stimulation.    The 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


745 


suppression  reappears  if  the  stimulus  is  reapplied 
peripherally  to  the  point  of  division  of  the  bundle. 
Rasmussen's  efferent  fibers  are  therefore  evidently 
capable  of  suppressing  activity  in  auditory  afferents 
either  at  or  near  the  point  of  impulse  origin  within  the 
cochlea. 

Optic  j\erve  Activity 

Granit,  by  stimulating  the  midbrain  tegmentum, 
induced  a  lasting  augmentation  of  the  frequency  of 
firing  of  individual  ganglion  cells  in  the  retina, 
whether  the  latter  were  spontaneously  active  or  were 
made  active  by  test  flash  illumination  (29).  Occasion- 
ally, from  the  same  general  region,  inhibition  is 
elicited.  Both  the  facilitatory  and  inhibitory  effects 
appear  to  be  conveyed  by  fine  efferent  fibers  described 
by  Ramon  \  Cajal  and  others  (e.g.  65,  vol.  II,  fig. 
211,  p.  366).  Dodt,  by  stimulating  the  optic  tract  in 
rabbits,  elicited  small,  late-appearing  retinal  spikes 
which  are  unlike  antidromic  spikes;  these  he  inter- 
preted as  due  to  impulses  conveyed  along  the  cen- 
trifugal fibers  to  the  retina  (15)-  The  exact  central 
origin  of  such  centrifugal  fibers  is  not  yet  determined. 
These  efferent  effects  upon  retinal  activity  are  most 
readily  initiated  by  stimulation  of  the  reticular  forma- 
tion of  the  midbrain  and  are  reliably  obtained  only  in 
animals  lacking  central  anesthesia  (29,  39). 

Olfactory  Bull)  Activity 

The  anterior  commissure  contains  cflerent  fibers, 
described  by  Ramon  y  Cajal  and  others  (e.g.  65,  vol. 
II,  p.  664,  figs.  423-425;  66,  p.  12),  which  apparently 
arise  in  basal  rhinencephalic  areas  and  pass  out  to  the 
olfactory  bulb.  These  and  similar  fibers  coming  from 
the  opposite  bulb  are  believed  to  terminate  on  granule 
cells  and  in  the  periventricular  and  external  plexiform 
layers.  In  this  location  they  have  access  to  the  synaptic 
junction  between  receptor-cell  terminals  and  bulbar 
neurons.  Kerr  &  Hagbarth  (46)  studied  the  effects  of 
exciting  this  centrifugal  system  upon  the  electrical 
activity  of  the  olfactory  bulb,  both  in  the  resting  state 
and  following  olfactory  stimulation.  Excitation  of  the 
anterior  commissure,  the  prepyriform  cortex,  the 
cortical  amygdaloid  nucleus  and  the  olfactory  tubercle 
induces  a  diminution  of  olfactory-bulb  activity.  Ef- 
ferent fibers  apparently  exercise  a  tonic  inhibitory 
influence  upon  the  olfactory  bulb  since  the  addition 
of  central  anesthesia  or  a  surgical  division  of  the  an- 
terior commissure  is  followed  by  an  augmentation  of 
olfactory-bulb  activity. 


CONTROL  OF  CENTRAL  SENSOR"!'  REL.AYS 

Spinal  Ascending  Relays 

Magoun  observed  in  1950  (58)  that  the  "study  of 
descending  influences  of  the  reticular  formation  has 
so  far  been  preoccupied  entirely  with  the  pronounced 
effects  exerted  upon  the  discharge  of  spinal  motor 
neurons.  It  would  be  of  considerable  interest  to 
know  whether  or  not  these  generalized  reticulospinal 
influences  are  capable  also  of  altering  the  transmission 
of  afferent  impulses  within  the  cord."  The  effect  of 
centrifugal  influences  upon  the  synaptic  relay  of  im- 
pulses from  dorsal  root  fibers  to  second  order  ascend- 
ing neurons  was  first  tested  by  Hagbarth  &  Kerr  in 
■954  (sO-  Using  cats  immobilized  with  curare  and 
lacking  central  anesthesia,  they  applied  test  shocks  to 
individual  lumbosacral  dorsal  roots  and  analyzed  the 
effects  of  intervening  (conditioning)  excitation  applied 
elsewhere  in  the  central  nervous  .system.  They  found 
that  stimulation  in  either  the  inhibitory  or  facilitatory 
zones  of  the  reticular  formation  diminishes  or  abol- 
ishes responses  being  conveyed  within  both  the  ventral 
and  lateral  funiculi  of  the  spinal  cord.  The  relayed 
response  in  the  dorsal  columns  is  also  affected  although 
the  primary  dorsal  column  spike,  representing  con- 
duction along  primary  afferent  fibers,  is  unaltered. 
Stimulation  of  a  number  of  other  parts  of  the  central 
nervous  system,  the  sensorimotor  cortex,  the  second 
somatic  sensory  area,  the  anterior  part  of  the  cingulate 
gyrus  and  the  anterior  vermis  of  the  cerebellum,  has 
similar  but  less  pronounced  effects.  An  example  of 
this  is  shown  in  figure  3. 

When  central  anesthetics  are  administered,  there  is 
an  augmentation  in  amplitude  of  the  relayed  re- 
sponse as  compared  to  preanesthetic  levels  (fig.  4). 
Additionally,  if  the  spinal  cord  is  divided  in  the  cer- 
vical region  in  animals  without  central  anesthesia,  a 
similar  'release'  appears,  resulting  in  an  increase  of 
amplitude  in  the  second  order  neuron  responses  to  a 
standard  dorsal-root  volley  (31).  Evidently  in  anes- 
thetized animals  the  high  amplitude  of  .sensory-e\oked 
responses  recorded  within  the  classical  sensory  path- 
wa\s  is  due  to  the  anesthetic  having  interrupted  a 
tonic  descending  inhibitory  influence. 

Dorsal  Column  and  Other  Bulbar  Relays 

Excitation  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation 
induces  a  prolonged  depression  of  transmission 
through  the  dorsal  column  relay  nuclei  (39,  71).  A 
moderately  intense  i-sec.  stimulation  causes  a  rapid 


746 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


onset  and  slow  decay  of  depression,  affecting  impulses 
being  relayed  to  the  internal  arcuate  fibers.  The  ini- 
tial spike  of  impulses  arri\-ing  via  the  dorsal  columns 
is  not  affected.  Not  only  is  the  relayed  response  of  the 
dorsal  column  nuclei  modified,  but  the  background 
activity  of  reticular  neurons  at  the  same  level  is  af- 
fected, although  with  a  different  time  course,  by  the 
same  conditioning  reticular  stimulation.  On  the  intro- 
duction of  central  anesthetics  or  the  production  of  a 
mid-line  pontine  lesion,  there  is  a  notable  increase  in 
amplitude  of  evoked  responses  pa.ssing  through  the 
dorsal-column  nuclei,  indicating  that  ordinarily  there 


"^^^^W^^^ 

'^«^'^^%/\.'^  -  -     - 


I  M  I  I  I  I  I  r  r 


I  r  I  r  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I 

10   msec. 


FIG.  3.  Cerebellar  inllucnces  on  .spinal  sensory  transmission. 
Responses  are  elicited  by  stimulation  of  the  right  dorsal  root 
L7,  and  recorded  from  the  left  ventral  column  of  the  spinal 
cord  {upper  beam')  and  left  sensory  cortex  (lower  beam)-  Inter- 
current stimulation  is  applied  to  the  ventral  part  of  the  anterior 
vermis  of  the  cerebellum.  Test  responses  recorded  (/)  prior  to 
cerebellar  stimulation,  (.?)  during  cerebellar  stimulation,  (j) 
I  sec.  and  (4)  3  sec.  after  termination  of  cerebellar  stimulation. 
Dorsal  columns  of  spinal  cord  were  sectioned  at  L4.  [From 
Hagbarth  &  Kerr  (31).] 


I     I     t     I     I     I     I 

5  m  sec . 


FIG.  4.  Release'  of  tonic  descending  inhibitory  influences  by 
anesthesia  and  by  cord  transection.  Curarized  cats  without 
central  anesthesia.  Top  row:  Left  ventral  column  response  to 
feeble  L7  dorsal  root  stimulation  (^4)  before,  and  (B)  after 
injection  of  45  mg  chloralose  per  kg.  BoUom  row :  Effect  of  high 
cord  section  on  left  ventral  column  response.  A  before,  and  B 
1  hr.  after  transection.  In  each  experiment  the  stimulus  in- 
tensity and  location  were  kept  constant;  the  dorsal  columns 
had  been  transected  at  the  L4  level.  [From  Hagbarth  &  Kerr 


(30-] 


is  a  tonic  descending  inhibitory  influence  acting  upon 
this  relay  station. 

Impulses  being  relayed  through  the  spinal  root  of 
the  trigeminal  nerve  in  response  to  test  shocks  applied 
to  the  ophthalmic  branch  of  the  trigeminal  are  also 
diminished  by  stimulation  of  the  brain-stem  reticular 
formation  (36).  Sensory-evoked  responses  in  the  adja- 
cent reticular  formation  are  sometimes  depressed  for 
more  than  a  minute  even  though  the  trigeminal  nu- 
clear response  is  only  transiently  affected.  Stimulation 
of  the  sensorimotor  cortex  will  also  bring  about  active 
inhibition  of  the  trigeminal  synaptic  relay,  as  appears 
in  figure  5. 

Jouvet  &  Dcsmedt  report  that  stimulation  of  the 
mesencephalic  reticular  forntation  will  cause  a  marked 
reduction  in  amplitude  of  auditor\-e\oked  responses 
recorded  from  the  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus  (44).  This 
occurs  even  when  the  electrical  responses  recorded 
from  the  round  window  in  response  to  the  same 
sensorv  stimuli   are  unaffected.   They  conclude  that 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


747 


0  m  sec 


peaks  of  facilitation,  holds  for  the  thalamic  relay  of 
c\'oked  responses  during  barbiturate  anesthesia  or 
following  lesions  placed  in  the  brain-stem  reticular 
formation.  These  alterations  result  from  the  'release' 
from  a  tonic  inhibitory  reticular  influence  which  evi- 
dently modulates  the  thalamic  relay  nuclei  during 
wakefulness  (48).  Other  evidence  indicates  that  stim- 
ulation of  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  will 
affect  the  lateral  geniculate  as  well  as  retinal  relays  of 
photically-evoked  responses  (39).  Apparently  evoked 
responses  to  the  same  flash  signal  may  be  augmented 
in  the  retina  and  yet  depressed  in  the  thalamus. 

Altogether,  these  experiments  suggest  that  each  of 
the  major  stations  which  relay  afferent  impulses 
within  the  spinal  cord,  medulla  and  thalamus  appears 
to  be  susceptible  to  interference  by  inhibitory  influ- 
ences, and  that  these  influences  are  tonically  active 
in  the  unanesthetized  animal. 


FIG.  5.  Sensorimotor  cortex  inHuence  on  trigeminal  relay 
and  brain-stem  reticular  formation  responses  to  infraorbital 
nerve  stimulation.  Curarized  cats  without  central  anesthesia. 
A.  Bulbar  recording  from  left  spinal  fifth  tract.  Afferent  tri- 
geminal response  following  stimulation  of  the  left  infraorbital 
nerve  (/)  before,  (2)  during,  (j)  3  sec.  after  and  (^)  6  sec.  after 
repetitive  stimulation  of  right  sensorimotor  cortex  (100  per 
sec.  for  3  sec).  In  this  record  the  trigeminal  response  is  com- 
posed mainly  of  a  secondary  wave,  the  primary  spike  being 
hardly  visible  as  an  initial  notch.  B.  Recording  from  the  right 
side  of  the  midbrain  reticular  formation.  Reticular  response 
evoked  by  infraorbital  stimulation  (i)  before,  (2)  13  sec.  after 
and  (3)  about  20  sec.  after  repetitive  stimulation  of  right  sen- 
sorimotor cortex  (100  per  sec.  for  3  sec).  [From  Hernandez- 
Peon  &  Hagbarth  (36).] 


the  inhibitory  effect  is  probaljly  taking  place  at  the 
level  of  the  central  (dorsal  cochlear  nucleu.s)  relay. 

Thalamic  Relays 

Recently  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  has 
been  found  capable  of  altering  synaptic  transmission 
through  thalamic  relay  nuclei.  In  animals  without 
central  anesthesia,  test-evoked  responses  being  con- 
veyed through  the  somatosensory  relay  (from  the 
medial  lemniscus  to  the  internal  capsule)  develop  a 
shortened  latency  and  duration  and  a  reduced  ampli- 
tude during  brain-stem  activation  (48).  The  peaks  of 
facilitation  that  otherwise  appear  during  recovery 
following  a  relayed  volley  are  likewi.se  obliterated. 
The  converse,  i.e.  a  long  latency,  high  amplitude  and 
prolonged  duration   response  followed   by  succes.sive 


CEPHALIC  INTERACTION  SYSTEMS 

Cortuipiial  Prnjulum  Systtmi 

In  addition  to  the  primary  somcsthetic  sensory  re- 
sponses which  are  highly  resistant  to  deep  anesthesia, 
there  are  the  so-called  'secondary'  responses  which 
have  longer  latency,  are  more  widespread  and  are 
somewhat  less  resistant  to  anesthesia  (5,  20).  These 
have  been  shown  to  be  independent  of  the  classical 
medial  lemniscus  pathway  and  to  be  dependent 
upon  structures  lying  in  the  medial  part  of  the 
cephalic  brain-stem  (14,  62).  These  secondary  re- 
sponses are  recorded  well  beyond  the  ijoundaries  of 
the  classical  somesthetic  receiving  cortex  and  may 
even  be  of  higher  amplitude  in  the  surrounding  asso- 
ciation cortex  (33,  42,  43,  76,  77,  79). 

A  number  of  additional  studies  have  extended  the 
analysis  of  the  somesthetic  secondary  response  and 
have  found  what  appear  to  be  analogous  .secondary 
responses  relating  to  the  auditory  and  visual  systeins 
as  well.  Recent  studies,  illustrated  in  figure  6,  of 
Buser  &  Borenstein  may  be  taken  as  exemplary  of 
current  insight  into  these  mechanisms  (i  i)."  Primary 

-  Recent  work  confirms  that  the  'secondary  discharge'  of 
Forbes  &  Morison  (20),  observed  in  rather  deeply  anesthetized 
cats,  probably  involves  a  different  mechanism  from  that  re- 
sponsible for  the  'reponses  sensorielles  secondaires'  of  Buser  cSi. 
Borenstein  (11),  observed  in  animals  lacking  central  anesthesia. 
Drs.  Evarts  and  Fleming  (personal  communication)  have 
established  that  by  recording  from  implanted  electrodes  in  the 
visual  receiving  cortex  of  the  cat  they  can  demonstrate  a  dis- 


748 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 


FIG.  6.  Cortical  zones  showing  reinforcement  of  responses 
between  two  heterogeneous  sensory  stimuh.  Cats  curarized, 
without  central  anesthesia  or  with  a  light  dose  of  chloralose. 
When  two  heterogeneous  stimuli  occur  nearly  simultaneously, 
certain  cortical  loci  outside  of  the  primary  receiving  areas 
show  an  interaction  between  the  two  stimuli  (potentially  the 
effect  is  either  facilitatory  or  inhibitory  but  is  most  readily  ident- 
ified if  facilitatory).  Auditory  and  somesthetic  reinforcement  in 
,-1,  visual  and  somesthetic  in  B;  auditory  and  visual  in  C.  The 
principal  primary  receiving  areas  are  outlined  by  dolled  lines. 
GSP,  posterior  sigmoid  gyrus;  C,  coronal  gyrus;  SSA  and  SSM, 
anterior  and  middle  suprasylvian  gyri;  LAT,  lateral  gyrus; 
ESM,  middle  ectosyl\ian  gyrus.  [From  Buser  &  Borenstein 
(")•] 


and  secondary  response  systems  arc  differendated  by 
being  a)  independent  in  pathway  (lemniscus  and 
medial  brain-stem  reticular  formation),  i)  different 
in  susceptibility  to  anesthetic  agents  (primary  re- 
sponses are  highly  resistant,  secondary  responses  are 
more  vulnerable  to  barbiturate  anesthesia),  <)  differ- 
ent in  latency  (for  primary  somesthetic  responses, 
approximately  8  to  lo  msec,  as  compared  with  those 
for  secondary  responses,  approximately  40  to  80  msec.) 
and  d)  different  in  areal  extent  on  cortex  (primary 
localized  to  classical  sensory  projection  area,  second- 
ary extending  widely  into  association  cortex  where 
the  modalities  belonging  to  the  different  sensory 
systems  overlap  each  other). 

There  is  an  important  functional  distinction   be- 
tween the  classical  sensory  pathways  and  the  ascend- 


continuity  of  secondary  responses  during  the  application  of 
increasing  increments  of  barbiturate  anesthesia.  An  early 
secondary  response  which  can  be  discerned  when  conditions 
are  favorable  in  the  completely  unanesthetized  cat  disappears 
with  light  stages  of  anesthesia  (pentobarbital,  15  mg/kg),  and 
a  much  larger  secondary  response  appears  at  a  deeper  stage  of 
anesthesia  (30  mg/kg)  and  after  a  substantially  longer  latency. 


ing  brain-stein  reticular  system.  French  &  Magoun 
(22)  found  that  monkeys  with  bilateral  destruction  of 
the  classical  lemniscal  pathways  in  the  midbrain  are 
still  aroused  from  sleep  by  sound  and  touch  stimuli. 
When  the  reticular  formation  in  the  midbrain  is 
destroyed,  however,  leaving  the  classical  ascending 
sensory  pathways  intact,  the  monkeys  remain  in  coma, 
even  though  sensory  evoked  potentials  can  be  re- 
corded in  the  auditory  and  somesthetic  receiving 
cortices.  Central  anesthetics  block  conduction  in 
certain  extralemniscal  pathways,  and  this  undoubt- 
edly represents  an  important  basis  for  their  action  as 
anesthetics  (23).  These  facts  underline  the  importance 
to  sensory  evoked  arousal,  and  presumably  to  sensa- 
tion in  general,  of  the  extralemniscal  pathways. 

Cortical  Intrrarlion  Sysli'ms 

High  frequency  stimulation  of  the  brain-stem  retic- 
ular formation  yields  a  generalized  reduction  in  de- 
gree of  synchronization  among  cortical  neurons  (63). 
The  effect  on  the  electrocorticographic  patterns  imi- 
tates the  desynchronization  that  takes  place  during 
natural  arousal.  It  has  been  shown  that  brain-stem 
activation  is  accompanied  by  an  increase  in  the  rate 
of  discharge  of  neurons  throughout  the  cephalic  brain- 
stem, including  the  diffusely  projecting  thalamic 
system  (57).  As  is  well  known,  almost  all  individual 
cortical  loci  are  reciprocally  related  to  points  that  are 
symmetrically  placed  on  the  opposite  hemisphere,  as 
though  in  mirror  image  of  each  other.  Chang  dis- 
covered, as  shown  in  figure  7,  that  when  one  records 
evoked  potentials  from  a  given  cortical  locus,  an 
intervening  stimulation  of  the  homotopically  related 
point  on  the  opposite  hemisphere  will  modify  the 
evoked  response  (12,  13).  von  Euler  &  Ricci  (81) 
have  analyzed  this  capacity  for  interference  with  pri- 
mary cortical  sensory-evoked  responses  on  the  part  of 
separate  cortical  inputs.  By  stimulating  the  classical 
thalamic  relay  nuclei  and  recording  the  primary 
evoked  cortical  responses,  these  investigators  could 
then  add  conditioning  stimuli  to  the  contralateral 
homotopic  cortical  point.  They  find,  as  did  C^hang, 
that  these  systems  converge  and  interact  within  the 
sensory  cortex  (81).  Afferent  impulses  arriving  in  the 
sensory  cortex  are  known  to  interact  there  with  non- 
specific impulses  from  the  thalamic  recruiting  system 
(43).  Moreover,  recruiting  responses  recorded  from 
the  cortex  are  found  to  be  altered  during  behavioral 
alerting  to  sound  stimuli  (18). 

All  of  these  facts  sulxstantiate  the  general  principle 
that  within  the  cortical  receiving  areas,  as  at  each  of 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


749 


% 

120 

- 

•• 

100 

•-- 

.  ••• 

••  •  • 

•    • 
• 

• 

• 

• 
• 
• 
• 

••  • 

• 
•    • 

• 
• 

•' 

• 

• 
•    • 

• 

• 

• 

• 

•• 

— ••... 

• 

•       • 

•    • 

80 

...,..^. 

• 
• 
• 

0          o 

o 

o 

60 

•  • 
• 
• 

0       0 

0 

0 

0                 ° 
0 

40 

• 

•• 
• 

o 

0 

0 

o 

o 
o 
o 

0 
0 

o 
oo 

o 

• 
•• 

0    0 
O          00 

0 

o 

O      0 

o 

0 

0 

0 

0 
0 

o 

0 

20 

O          O        0 

0 

0 

0 

oo       o 

0  o 

o 

•O       0 

°      °           °Oo 

o    o 

o 

0 

o- 

1  n            1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

t 

1 

20  40  60  80  100  120  140  160  i80        200 


msec. 


FIG.  7.  Time  course  of  blocking  effect  of  callosal  potential  on  positive  and  negative  components 
of  primary  auditory  response.  Abscissae:  Time  after  delivery  of  stimulus  on  contralateral  cortex 
for  callosal  potential.  Ordinatcs:  Magnitude  of  auditory  response  expressed  as  percentage  of  control. 
Dots  represent  positive  component  and  circles,  negative  component.  Note  that  stimulation  of  the 
contralateral  homotopic  cortical  locus  modifies,  by  its  callosal  connections,  the  sensory-evoked 
response  in  a  primary  receiving  area.  [From  Chang  (13).] 


the  Other  stations  along  the  sensory  pathways,  the 
character  and  e.xtent  of  sensory-evoked  responses  are 
subject  to  intervention  by  activities  taking  place  else- 
where within  widespread  regions  of  the  brain. 

Corticifugal  Influences  on  Brain-stem  Mechanisms 

Bremer  &  Terzuolo  (9)  showed  that  stimulation  of 
the  cortex  in  cats  without  central  anesthesia  will  in- 
duce electrocorticographic  evidence  of  arousal.  Jasper 
and  co-workers  (41),  working  with  monkeys,  had 
earlier  shown  a  spread  of  localized  cortically-induced 
after-discharges  into  the  brain  stem.  It  has  subse- 
quently been  observed,  in  monkeys  without  central 
anesthesia,  that  single  shock  stimuli  delivered  to  spe- 
cific regions  of  the  cortex  will  yield  evoked  potentials 
throughout  a  wide  zone  of  the  cephalic  brain  stem 
(1-3,  21).  This  zone  is  generally  coextensive  with  the 
brain-stem  region  within  which  sensory  responses  from 
different  sensory  systems  appear  to  converge.  Blocking 
and  facilitating  interaction  takes  place  in  this  general 
region  among  the  various  combinations  of  cortically 
and    peripherally   initiated   signals   (8,    21,    36,    70). 


Examples  appear  in  figures  8  and  9.  The  corticifugal 
projections  not  only  interact  with  other  signals  con- 
verging upon  the  brain  stem  but  also  with  signals 
intrinsic  to  the  reticular  formation,  i.e.  signals  gen- 
erated within  and  recorded  from  the  brain-stem  retic- 
ular formation  itself  (2),  as  may  be  seen  in  figure  10. 
The  same  corticifugal  systems  are  known  to  be  capable 
of  initiating  electrocorticographic  (73)  and  behav- 
ioral C72)  arousal,  presumably  by  virtue  of  their  con- 
nections with  the  cephalic  brain-stem  reticular  forma- 
tion. 

It  can  be  seen  that  not  only  are  input  and  output 
patterns  modifiable  within  the  cortex,  but  the  cortex 
itself  can  also  modify  activity  taking  place  within  the 
ijrain  stem  and  thereby  possibly  have  an  indirect  in- 
fluence back  upon  sensory  patterns  as  these  are  ini- 
tiated and  relayed  at  lower  levels. 

Organization  of  Centrifugal  Sensory  Control  Mechanisms 

Up  to  this  point  we  have  described  mainly  how 
activity  in  each  of  the  sensory  neurons  linking  to- 
gether a  given  classical  ascending  sensory  pathway 


750 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


i*iiAMi^**«i*MMiitiii*iita^*iMMlMMi^i^MW«M«4i 


sAAAAAAAAA/VWNAAA/VX/WWXAAy 

lioo^V  25/sec  noo^V 


FIG.  8.  Inhibition  of  reticular  unit  discharges  by  stimulation 
of  the  cerebellum  and  augmentation  of  reticular  unit  dis- 
charges by  sensory  stimulation.  Encfphale  isote  preparation, 
without  cential  anesthesia.  For  each  strip,  the  top  line  registers 
an  electroencephalographic  tracing  recorded  from  the  fronto- 
temporal  regions  of  a  hemisphere;  the  lower  line  indicates  unitary 
spike  discharges  picked  up  from  the  median  bulbar  reticular 
formation  by  a  microelectrode.  A.  .Activated  electroencephalo- 
gram and  continuous  high  frequency  discharge  of  the  reticular 
unit.  B.  Total  inhibition  of  the  unit  by  positive  polarization  of 
the  anterior  vermis  of  the  cerebellum  (0.5  ma);  EEG  trace  not 
modified.  C.  Immediately  after  discontinuation  of  the  cere- 
bellar polarization.  D.  Same  as  C  a  few  seconds  later.  Tlie 
reticular  unit  reappears  (C)  and  progressively  increases  in 
discharge  frequency  (D);  during  the  early  part  of  this  period, 
slow  high-amplitude  waves  appear  in  the  EEG.  E.  Some 
minutes  later,  an  intense  tactile  stimulation  (brisk  rubbing  of 
the  bridge  of  the  nose)  causes  the  appearance  of  a  multiple 
reticular  discharge  (including  recruitment  of  new  units),  and 
an  increase  in  frequency  of  EEG  waves.  [From  Mollica  et  at. 
(6.).] 


is  subject  to  some  degree  of  interference  according  to 
the  state  of  activity  in  other  parts  of  the  nervous 
system.  Now,  is  it  possible  to  define  somewhat  more 
specifically  the  relationship  between  the  classical 
sensory  paths  and  these  other  parts?  No  final  inter- 
pretations are  warranted  since  the  data  are  as  yet 
incomplete  for  any  one  sensory  system.  Nonetheless, 
in  each  sensory  system  there  can  be  identified  certain 
centrifugal  sensory  control  mechanisms  which  bear 
close  analogy  with  structural  or  functional  aspects  of 
one  or  another  of  the  other  sensory  systems.  Generali- 
zations that  might  not  be  permitted  for  one  system 
alone  seem  to  gain  in  strength  when  all  of  them  are 
examined  together. 

Paralleling    the    classical    succession    of   ascending 
neurons  appears  a  descending  system  which  links  the 


same  nuclear  relay  stations  from  above  downward. 
Although  analogous  centrifugal  projections  have  been 
identified  anatomically  for  many  individual  parts  of 
other  sensory  systems,  the  auditory  pathway  probably 
po.ssesses  the  most  completely  documented  succession 
of  descending  fibers.  These  pursue  a  course  in  reverse 
direction  that  roughly  parallels  the  ascending;  audi- 
tory pathway.  They  pass  step-by-step  downward  from 
the  auditory  cortex  to  the  medial  geniculate  body  and 
inferior  colliculus,  thence  to  the  lateral  lemniscus  and 
trapezoid  body  and  to  the  superior  olive  where  they 
are  succeeded  by  the  olivocochlear  efferent  bundle. 
As  Galambos  says,  "It  is  unlikeh  that  these  descend- 
ing fiber  .systems — some  reasonably  powerful,  some 
weak — perform  no  function  in  audition.  What  this 
function  might  be  will  unfortunately  continue  to  re- 
main entireK'  speculati\e  until  more  anatomical  and 
physiological  data  become  available.  One  can  hazard 
a  guess,  however,  that  the  solution  of  certain  problems 
of  hearing  resides  as  much  in  the  understanding  of 
the  function  of  these  descending  pathways  as  in  the 
knowledge  of  the  ascending  ones"  (25,  p.  503).  Pre- 
sumably centrifugal  fiber  projections  which  belong  to 
the  visual,  somesthetic  and  olfactorv  svstems  mia;ht 


B 


W*/<-^; 


10  m  sec 


10  nri  sec 


FIG.  9.  Influence  of  hypothalamic  and  toe  pad  stimulation 
on  sciatic  nerve  responses  elicited  within  the  brain-stem  reticu- 
lar formation.  Recording  from  the  right  side  of  the  midbrain 
reticular  formation  in  a  curarized  cat  without  central  anes- 
thesia. A.  Sciatic  responses,  (/)  before,  (.?)  during,  (3)  8  sec. 
after  and  (^)  20  sec.  after  repetitive  stimulation  in  the  right 
hypothalamic  region  (50  per  sec.  for  3  sec).  B.  Sciatic  response 
(/)  before,  (s)  during  and  (3)  10  sec.  after  pinching  the  toe  pads 
of  right  hind  limb  (ipsilateral  to  reticular  recording  site). 
[From  Hernandez-Peon  &  Hagbarth  (36).] 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


751 


STIMULATION 
POINT   "A" 


'^/'^^''»'''■'%^^*^ 


RETICULAR 
RESPONSE 
POSTERIOR 

TO    ANTERIOR 


RETICULAR 
RESPONSE    TO 
CORTICAL 
STIMULATION 


MULATtON 
POINT    "B" 


SHOCK 
INTERVALS 


4    MS 


12 


20 


25 


31 


39 


48 


59 


93 


rv/'^flv^Nw^ 


D^V 


*\Nv^ 


\|L/Vv,sM****M 


»/|^,.^S,.VSv^ 


' /V,^^>^''**^ 


'1    J-^^-mA^N^ 


^^^M-^-H^^*^ 


^^/^v^***'*N 


*^v^ 


COMBINED 
CORTICAL 

a 

RETICULAR 
STIMULATION 


SHOCK 

INTLRvaLS 


4    MS 


[yx/'^s^ 


p/w^*^ 


p^/^-^^-^,- 


/y/irv^ 


fW^.^^^S.^PV-^Mi* 


20 


25 


31 


39 


48 


59 


93 


FIG.  10.  Coiticifugal  inHuences 
upon  a  conduction  pathway  in 
the  brain-stem  reticular  forma- 
tion. Responses  recorded  from 
bipolar  electrodes  in  the  anterior 
brain  stem  show  effects  of  single 
cortical  shocks  on  volleys  ascend- 
ing from  a  test  stimulation  site 
in  the  posterior  brain  stem.  Left 
column:  Effects  of  cortical  shocks 
applied  to  point  'A'  on  the 
medial  surface  of  the  monkey 
hemisphere.  Right  column:  Effects 
on  the  same  pathway  of  shocks 
applied  to  a  more  anterior 
cortical  site,  point  'B".  Note  that 
ascending  brain-stem  volley  is 
facilitated  when  cortical  shock  is 
delivered  to  point  'A'  31  msec. 
prior  to  posterior  brain  stem  test 
shock,  whereas  facilitation  from 
point  'B'  occurs  at  9  and  again 
at  48  msec,  at  which  moments 
the  brain-stem  pathway  is  being 
inhibited  from  point  'A'.  This 
illustrates  the  principle  that  a 
number  of  cortical  sites  can  exert 
a  controlling  influence  on  ascend- 
ing systems  intrinsic  to  the  brain 
stem,  thereby  being  able,  pre- 
sumably, to  interfere  with  mecha- 
nisms involved  in  sensation. 
[From  Adey  el  at.  (i).] 


752 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


play  an  analogously  important,  but  as  yet  undefined, 
role  in  the  perception  of  each  of  these  modalities. 

It  is  generally  accepted  that  the  classical  ascending 
sensory  pathways  connect  directly  or  indirectly  with 
the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  (78).  Rasmussen 
reports  that  the  centrifugal  auditory  projection  sys- 
tem also  sends  branches  into  the  same  general  region 
(personal  communication).  It  may  be  by  virtue  of 
such  collateral  connections  to  the  brain-stem  reticular 
formation  that  most  sensory  pathways  and  certain 
zones  of  the  cerebral  cortex  have  indirect  reciprocal 
relations  with  the  cerebellum  (6,  32,  74,  75).  Each  of 
the  cerebral  and  cerebellar  cortical  areas  which  is 
capable  of  exercising  an  eflfect  upon  impulses  trans- 
mitted along  the  classical  ascending  sensory  pathways 
possesses  projections  to  the  brain-stem  reticular  for- 
mation. Moreover,  stimulation  of  the  brain-stem  re- 
ticular formation  is  known  to  evoke  very  notable 
sensorv  control  effects.  This  much  is  highly  suggestive. 
But  whether  the  descending  sensory  projections  which 
parallel  the  classical  ascending  sensory  pathways  and 
the  cerebral  and  cereljellar  projections  into  the  brain 
stem  each  have  an  influential  access  to  the  ascending 
sensory  transmission  systems  must  await  definitive 
experimental  proof. 

To  these  considerations  should  be  added  the  fact 
that  there  is  really  no  physiological  boundary  between 
central  sensory  and  motor  mechanisms.  Each  central 
pattern  for  the  initiation  of  movement  has  its  neuronal 
repercussions  upon  central  sensory  patterns,  and  each 
performed  movement  introduces  alterations  in  sensory 
input  patterns.  In  this  way  sensory  and  motor  systems 
are  inextricably  bound  together  both  internally  and 
externally. 

Transactional  Mechanisms  Relating  to 
Sensory  Control  Systems 

It  is  now  possible  to  identify  six  extensive,  mutually 
interacting  systems:  a)  the  classical  ('lemniscal') 
ascending  sensory  pathways  projecting  finally  upon 
the  classical  sensory  receiving  areas  of  the  cortex,  b~) 
the  parallel  ('extralemniscal')  ascending  sensory 
pathways  which  reach  more  widespread  regions  of 
the  cortex  by  way  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  forma- 
tion, f)  the  classical  ('pyramidal')  descending  motor 
pathways  projecting  directly  from  cortex  to  lower 
motoneuron  aggregations,  ctj  the  parallel  ('extra- 
pyramidal') motor  pathways  which  descend  to  the 
motor  nuclei  indirectly  by  way  of  the  basal  ganglia 
and  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation,  e)  the  brain- 
stem  reticular   formation   which   is   known   to   exert 


modifying  influences  upward  upon  both  the  cerebral 
and  cerebellar  hemispheres  and  downward  upon  both 
sensory  and  motor  synaptic  relays,  and  /)  the  cen- 
trifugal sensory  control  mechanisms  which  may  in- 
volve fibers  coursing  in  reverse  direction  parallel  to 
the  classical  ascending  sensory  pathways  and  which 
may  also  implicate  projections  from  cerebral  and 
cerebellar  loci  through  the  brain-stem  reticular  for- 
mation. 

The  interdependence  of  these  six  systems  is  obvious. 
Evidently  they  are  all  knit  together  by  the  brain-stem 
reticular  formation  which  could  not  be  efTectively 
studied  in  animals  with  central  anesthesia.  Because 
of  this  experimental  limitation,  antecedent  concep- 
tions had  to  deal  with  relatively  independent  sensory 
and  motor  systems  which  were  more  stable,  imperious 
and  reliable  in  their  handling  of  signals  than  is  the 
case  in  the  unanesthetized  brain. 

Since  collaterals  from  the  classical  ascending 
pathways  influence  the  reticular  formation  and  the 
reticular  formation  in  turn  modifies  the  initiation  and 
transmission  of  impulses  along  the  classical  sensory 
pathways,  since  both  of  these  systems  interact  with 
each  other  again  in  the  sensory  receiving  cortex, 
since  the  reticular  formation  by  way  of  the  diffusely 
projecting  thalamic  nuclei  modifies  activity  generally 
throughout  the  cortex  and  the  cortex  in  turn  modifies 
activity  within  the  reticular  formation,  since  the  cere- 
bellum is  similarly  linked  both  ways  with  the  brain- 
stem reticular  formation,  etc.,  one  can  begin  to 
visualize  the  extent  of  abstraction  imposed  by  the 
experimental  isolation  of  only  a  few  elements  of  this 
entire  complex.  Moreover,  it  is  not  possible  to  de- 
fine how  such  a  'transactional  mechanism'  (55)  might 
operate  on  the  basis  of  any  single  experimental  ap- 
proach. By  adding  evidence  from  studies  that  in- 
corporate both  neurophysiological  and  behavioral 
techniques,  it  is  possible  to  add  a  new  dimension 
to  the  conception  of  the  mechanisms  involved  in 
the  central  control  of  sensorv  transmission. 


SENSORY    ATTENTION,    HABITU.ATION    AND    CONDITIONING 

Auditory  Habituation 

By  means  of  electrodes  implanted  within  the  dorsal 
cochlear  nucleus,  Galambos,  Hernandez-Peon  and 
their  associates  have  recorded  potentials  elicited  by 
acoustic  stimulation  during  the  course  of  behavioral 
studies  on  unanesthetized  cats.  Responses  to  the  same 
tone  pip  show  modest  fluctuations  in  amplitude  and 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


753 


CONTROL  -^■fft^^y-^  /r^-y^J^'     /^,■'/v^-V./'  ,Vv/>V 

CLICKS  ALONE  J 


HABITUATION    ,■  v*'>u,i  i   /iv«.,r  «.     jA    ,m   .  . 

CLICKS    ALDNE'^-^^''^W*^^-^^'  ^Vs-^/^^.>V 

AFTER    600              ,-,«..•  v      , 

CLICK- SHOCKS''*.'^^**^  ^-^^.A^I^V  •'/^aN^",>/. ,vv^^ 


DEHABITllATION                 '   .  ' 

AFTER    1100      /V-^-Wj".'      '^v"-^  V^V^.^vV-yVA/ 

CLICK  -SHOCKS                '  -• 

REHABITUATION 

AFTER    1800     >'-V^V-Nwir-JV*V.';  •''.v^N/-.a/rAfwwV^ 

CLICKS  ALONE 


0.9   SEC. 


SO>iV 


FIG.  II.  Modification  of  amplitude  of  click-evoked  responses 
in  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus  according  to  experience  of  the 
animal.  Unanesthetized  cat  with  recording  electrodes  im- 
planted in  the  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus.  Amplitude  of  responses 
elicited  by  clicks  repeated  every  second  gradually  declines  over 
many  trials.  The  animal  exhibits  behavioral  as  well  as  electro- 
physiological evidence  of  habituation  to  the  click  stimuli.  After 
habituation,  if  weak  electric  shocks  are  applied  to  the  foreleg 
of  the  same  animal  in  temporal  association  with  the  clicks,  the 
click -evoked  responses  gradually  become  increased  in  ampli- 
tude. The  dehabituation'  can  occur  within  only  a  few  trials  if 
sufficiently  powerful  shock  stimuli  are  applied,  as  shown  by 
Galambos  et  al.  (27).  The  dehabituation'  is  plastic  in  the  sense 
that  the  amplitude  of  the  click -evoked  responses  declines  once 
more  after  the  shock  stimuli  are  discontinued.  This  kind  of 
modification  of  sensory-evoked  responses  has  been  taken  as  an 
objective  evidence  of  conditioning.  [Modified  from  Jouvet  & 
Hernandez-Peon  (45).] 


undergo  periods  of  waxing  and  waning.  If  the  same 
tone  signal  is  repeated  many  times,  the  amplitude  of 
the  evoked  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus  responses  tends 
gradually  to  become  reduced  to  a  new  lower  level, 
although  the  fluctuations  still  persist  (27,  37,  38,  45). 
The  authors  refer  to  this  as  auditory  'adaptation'  or 
'habituation.'  If  the  tone  is  shifted  up  or  down  in 
pitch,  the  ev'oked  potentials  return  to  a  higher  ampli- 
tude once  more,  but  rehabituation  can  be  established 
to  the  new  tone  signal.  After  habituation  to  a  particu- 
lar tone  has  been  thoroughly  established  and  the  tone 
is  then  associated  with  a  nearly  simultaneous  signal, 
such  as  an  electric  shock  to  the  foreleg  or  across  the 
chest,   a   high  amplitude  cochlear  response  will   re- 


appear. This  has  been  referred  to  as  'dehabituation' 
(35).  After  discontinuation  of  the  electric  shock,  a  slow 
rehabituation  to  the  auditory  signal  takes  place  (27, 
45),  as  is  shown  in  figure  1 1 . 

Auditory  Conditioning 

These  fluctuations  in  amplitude  of  the  responses 
recorded  within  the  first  central  relay  stages  along  the 
auditory  pathway  may  be  reflected  by  roughly  parallel 
shifts  in  the  animal's  behavior.  When  first  introduced 
to  the  test-tone  signals,  an  animal  attentively  alerts  to 
each  tone  pip.  As  electrographic  evidence  of  habitua- 
tion occurs,  the  animal  shows  less  behavioral  evidence 
of  devoting  attention  to  the  acoustic  signals.  When 
habituated  and  then  given  an  unconditioned  electric 
shock  in  association  with  the  tone  signals,  the  animal 
behaves  as  if  it  has  suddenly  acquired  an  increased 
interest  in  the  associated  tone.  Growth  in  behavioral 
evidence  of  attention  usually  takes  place  a  few  trials 
in  ad\ance  of  the  growth  in  amplitude  of  the  evoked 
dorsal  cochlear  response,  but  the  modified 
(conditioned)  cochlear  response  lasts  approximately 
as  long,  during  e.\tinction  trials,  as  the  overtly  ex- 
pressed attention.  The  electrophysiological  plasticity 
in  response  of  the  nervous  system  has  with  some  justi- 
fication been  taken  as  an  objective  indication  of  con- 
ditioning. 

Using  electrical  shocks  applied  across  the  chests  of 
cats,  Galambos  et  al.  (27)  have  found  that  only  ten 
or  twentv  such  unconditioned  stimuli,  applied  in  asso- 
ciation with  clicks  to  which  the  cats  had  previously 
become  thoroughly  habituated,  were  sufficient  to 
cause  electrographic  as  well  as  behavioral  evidence  of 
conditioning.  Simultaneous  recordings  made  in  the 
cochlear  nucleus,  auditory  cortex,  hippocampus, 
septal  area  and  head  of  the  caudate  nucleus  show  that 
electrographic  changes  associated  with  this  kind  of 
conditioning  may  occur  at  several  difTerent  levels 
along  the  auditory  pathway  and  in  regions  other  than 
the  classical  auditory  system.  Cycles  of  associated  be- 
havioral and  electrophysiological  evidence  for  condi- 
tioning and  extinction  can  apparently  be  repeated 
indefinitely.  [Galambos  et  al.  (27)  may  be  consulted 
for  additional  evidence  and  commentary  on  condi- 
tioning in  relation  to  modifications  of  electrical 
activity  in  the  brain.  This  subject  is  also  discussed  by 
Galambos  in  Chapter  LXI  of  this  work.] 

Shifts  of  Attention 

Recording  from  electrodes  implanted  in  the  dorsal 
cochlear  nucleus  of  the  cat  before  habituation  had 


754 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


been  established,  Hernandez-Peon  et  al.  (38)  have 
tested  the  effects  on  auditory-evoked  potentials  of 
distraction  by  visual  and  olfactory  stimuli.  As  may  be 
seen  in  figure  12,  when  mice  in  ajar  are  placed  before 
the  experimental  animals,  or  when  fish  odors  are 
blown  into  the  cage  through  a  tube,  the  formerly  high 
amplitude  auditory  responses  are  immediately  re- 
duced in  amplitude.  The  effect  is  as  if  the  cats  had 
suddenly  shifted  from  a  naive  to  an  'habituated'  state 
with  reference  to  the  auditory  test-signal  responses. 
But  when  the  mice  are  removed  or  the  odor  blowing 
stopped,  and  after  the  cats  are  apparently  relaxed 
once  more,  the  evoked  auditory  potentials  return 
again  to  their  initial  high  level  of  amplitude.  The 
duration  of  the  reduced  auditory  potentials  corre- 
sponds closely  with  the  period  when  the  animals  are 
distracted  by  the  nonacoustic  signals. 

Visual  Responses 

Using  electrodes  implanted  in  the  brain-stem  reticu- 
lar formation,  optic  tract,  lateral  geniculate  body  and 
optic  radiation,  Hernandez-Peon  et  al.  (39)  were  able 
to  analyze  the  effects  of  intercurrent  brain-stem  stimu- 
lation on  the  relay  of  flash-evoked  responses  through 
the  retina  and  lateral  geniculate  body.  When  light 
flashes  are  reiterated  over  an  extended  period  of  time. 


the  flash-evoked  responses  at  each  point  along  the 
visual  pathway  tend  to  diminish  in  amplitude.  This 
suggests  that  there  is  a  mechanism  of  habituation 
operating  in  the  visual  system.  .Stimulation  of  the 
brain-stem  reticular  formation  or  behavioral  distrac- 
tion by  nonvisual  stimuli  is  associated  with  a  reduc- 
tion in  amplitude  of  the  nonhabituatcd  photic  re- 
sponse. 

Behavior  and  Xeurophyswlogy 

Although  these  studies  are  quite  recent,  and  only 
a  few  aspects  of  a  potentially  very  rich  field  have  been 
touched  upon,  certain  features  merit  special  comment. 
It  is  evident  that  the  activity  taking  place  along  at 
least  the  auditory  and  visual  pathways,  and  possibly 
the  olfactory  and  somesthetic  sensory  systems  as  well, 
is  vulnerable  to  systematic  intervention  in  accordance 
with  previous  experience  (habituation)  and  shift  of 
attention  (distraction)  (11,  45  and  other  pertinent 
chapters  in  19).  It  is  inferred,  but  not  yet  firmly  estab- 
lished, that  these  dynamic  changes  in  activity  within 
the  sensory  paths  are  accomplished  by  some  mecha- 
nism involving  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation. 
The  evidence  is  as  follows:  «)  activation  of  the  i)rain- 


FIG.  12.  Modification  of  click-evoked 
responses  in  the  cochlear  nucleus  during 
'attention'  in  the  unanesthetized  cat.  Re- 
cording from  implanted  electrodes  in 
the  dorsal  cochlear  nucleus  in  an  ani- 
mal prior  to  habituation  to  click  signals 
delivered  every  second.  Photographs  are 
simultaneous  vi'ith  potential  recordings 
opposite.  Top  and  bottom :  Cat  is  relaxed; 
the  click  responses  are  large.  Middle: 
While  the  cat  is  visually  attentive  to 
mice  in  a  jar,  the  click  responses  are 
diminished  in  amplitude.  [From  Her- 
nandez-Peon el  al.  (38).] 


,...,.,-.^^,,^,^.,^,.,...-v,^      -v.-^^V'^WJ^y^^- 


■■Xy-v^ 


,V.^^^,X'VvAv- 


'■ — y  ■ 

[lOOuv 


.^V^y/M^^^- 


,y»i-»M 


y^, 


0  500 

t_i ■  •  '  • 

M  SEC 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORV    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


755 


Stem  reticular  formation  in  unanesthetized  animals 
has  an  effect  on  nonhabituated  acoustic-  or  flash- 
evoked  responses  that  is  similar  to  distraction  of  atten- 
tion by  extraneous  stimulation  of  the  same  animals; 
A)  distraction  of  attention  by  extraneous  sensory  stimu- 
lation may  very  likely  have  its  effect,  as  does  arousal, 
by  activation  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation; 
f)  animals  given  barbiturate  anesthesia  (known  to 
interfere  with  activity  in  the  brain-stem  reticular  for- 
mation) cannot  be  habituated;  d)  if  animals  are 
habituated  prior  to  being  anesthetized,  the  sensory- 
evoked  potentials  change  from  habituated  (reduced) 
amplitude  to  the  initial  prehabituated  height  but 
typical  habituated  responses  reappear  following  re- 
covery from  the  anesthetic;  e)  in  habituated  animals, 
a  lesion  restricted  to  the  pontine  or  mesencephalic 
brain-stem  reticular  formation  is  followed  by  perma- 
nent 'release'  from  the  habituated  pattern.'' 

Recently,  Fuster  (24)  has  reported  that  monkeys 
trained  to  do  difficult  tachistoscopic  discriminations 
between  two  similar  objects  show  improved  perform- 
ance in  both  their  speed  of  response  and  percentage 
of  correct  choices  when  the  test  exposure  is  preceded 
by  a  very  brief  electrical  shock  applied  to  the  mesen- 
cephalic brain-stem  reticular  formation.  More  pro- 
longed stimulation  in  the  same  brain-stem  location 
interferes  deleteriously  with  both  the  reaction  time 
and  percentage  of  correct  choices.  These  findings 
imply  an  alteration  of  visual  sensory  or  possibly  judg- 
mental processes  as  a  result  of  brain-stem  activation. 
Although  there  is  no  way  of  being  certain,  in  Fuster's 
experiments,  where  the  effect  takes  place,  it  is  possible 
that  such  changes  occur  within  the  first  few  synapses 
along  the  visual  pathway.  This  might  be  inferred  from 
the  experiments  of  Granit  (29)  and  Hernandez-Peon 
et  al.  (39)  cited  above.  More  convincing  evidence  for 
improvement  in  the  kind  of  differentiation  demanded 
by  tachistoscopic  discrimination  is  reflected  in  experi- 
ments by  Lindsley  (53).  He  finds  that  two  flashes 
which  are  placed  close  enough  together  to  produce  a 

^  .\  note  of  caution.  Much  has  been  learned  within  the  last 
few  years  which  assigns  important  functions  to  the  brain-stem 
reticular  formation.'  It  must  be  remembered,  however,  that 
this  region  may  well  contain  several  functional  systems.  The 
studies  of  Adey,  Amassian,  Haugen,  Moruzzi  and  their  asso- 
ciates imply  that  this  is  the  case  (2,  8,  34,  47,  61).  In  the  first 
stages  of  interpreting  the  functions  of  so  large  and  complex  a 
region  of  the  brain,  it  is  natural  that  somewhat  overgcneralized 
and  sweeping  conclusions  may  be  alluring.  This  does  not  deny 
the  reliability  of  observations  made  to  date  but  implies  that, 
when  this  complex  skein  of  reticular  neurons  becomes  better 
understood,  a  greater  precision  in  the  localization  and  charac- 
terization of  its  functions  may  be  possible. 


single  large-humped  electrical  wave  in  the  lateral 
geniculate  body  are,  on  stimulation  of  the  brain-stem 
reticular  formation,  separated  into  a  two-peaked 
himip. 

Taken  as  a  whole,  all  of  these  behavioral  experi- 
ments reinforce  the  neurophysiological  evidence  that 
the  sensory  pathways  are  relatively  plastic  rather  than 
fi.xed  in  the  transmission  of  impulses  generated  by  a 
particular  stimulus.  Sensory  transmission  is  apparently 
modifiable  in  accordance  with  waking  experience. 
Moreover,  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  evi- 
dently plays  an  important  role  in  the  government  of 
such  neuronal  plasticity. 


I\'TERPRET.\TIONS 

Remarkable  changes  take  place  within  sensory 
circuits  when  one  shifts  from  the  u.se  of  anesthetized 
animals  to  animals  without  central  anesthesia.  In  the 
anesthetized  state  the  classical  sensory  pathways  con- 
vey high  amplitude  .signals  with  great  reliability  and 
consistency,  and  there  is  little  activity  within  the 
brain-stem  reticular  formation.  Clortical  responses  to 
sensory  stimuli  are  greatly  amplified  and  tend  to  be 
confined  to  the  classical  sensory  receiving  areas.  In 
the  waking  brain,  without  central  anesthesia,  the 
classical  sensory  pathways  convey  signals  that  are  less 
reproducible  from  one  moment  to  the  next.  Indeed, 
over  a  period  of  some  minutes  or  hours  there  may  be 
remarkable  alterations  in  the  size  of  evoked  responses 
to  a  given  stimulus.  In  addition,  there  are  widespread 
responses  elicited  throughout  extensive  cortical  and 
subcortical  regions.  It  seems  obvious  now  that  the 
classical  sensory  pathways  and  cortical  projection  sys- 
tems, no  matter  how  necessary  they  might  be  to  per- 
ception, are  not  in  themselves  .sufficient  for  perception. 

The  extralemniscal  sensory  pathways,  coursing 
through  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  and 
diffusely  projecting  thalainic  nuclei,  appear  to  have  a 
general  function  of  providing  an  integrative  back- 
ground or  context  for  perception.  Their  contribution 
in  this  respect  may  be  likened  usefully  to  the  organi- 
zational contribution  in  movement  and  behavior  that 
is  made  by  the  descending  extrapyramidal  projec- 
tions. They  may  be  thought  to  provide  a  general 
.sensory  awareness  and  feeling  tone  comparable  to  the 
background  of  excitability  and  motor  tone  generated 
by  the  extrapyramidal  system.  Nonetheless,  they  may 
convey  more  specific  sense  data  too.  Haugen  & 
Melzack  (34),  for  example,  report  persuasive  evidence, 
soine  of  which  appears  in  figures  13,  14  and  15,  that 


756  HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY  ^  NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  13.  Short  and  medium  latency  response  areas  of  brain  stem  and  thalamus  responding  to 
tooth  pulp  stimulation.  Narrow  vertical  stripes  identify  the  trigeminal  lemniscus  (medial  lemniscus) 
which  has  a  short  latency  and  a  dominantly  contralateral  projection.  Horizontal  stripes  mark  the 
trigeminobulbothalamic  path  (spinobulbothalamic),  with  short  latency  and  bilateral  projections. 
Stippled  areas  mark  the  ascending  portion  of  the  central  tegmental  fasciculus  (dorsal  secondary 
trigeminal  pathway),  with  medium  latency  and  bilateral  projection.  Diagonal  stripes  designate  the 
Eiscending  path  within  the  central  grey  which  possesses  medium  latency  and  bilateral  projections. 
The  reticular  formation  yields  widespread  responses  characterized  by  long  latency  and  bilateral 
character.  The  relevant  structures  are  indicated  on  the  leftside  of  each  level.  Recording  sites  (except 
within  reticular  formation)  are  shaded  on  the  right  side.  Abbreviations :  BC,  brachium  conjunctivum; 
BIC,  brachium  of  inferior  colliculus;  CE,  centralis;  CG,  central  grey;  CM,  center  median;  CTF, 
central  tegmental  fasciculus;  DBC,  decussation  of  brachium  conjunctivum;  DM,  dorsalis  medialis; 
H,  habenula;  HIP,  habenulointerpeduncular  tract;  IP,  interpeduncularis;  LG,  lateral  geniculate, 
LP,  lateralis  posterior;  MG,  medial  geniculate;  ML,  medial  lemniscus;  MLF,  medial  longitudinal 
fasciculus;  .\''/?,red  nucleus;  Pet/., peduncle;  PL,  parafascicularis;  Pul.,  pulvinar;  P]\  periventricular 
area;  Py,  pyramidal  tract;  SBT,  spinobulbothalamic  tract;  .S'C,  superior  colliculus;  .S'.V,  substantia 
nigra;  Sbf.,  subparafascicularis;  STh,  subthalamicus,  TBT,  trigeminobulbothalamic  tract;  TL, 
trigeminal  lemniscus;  TO,  optic  tract;  VL,  ventralis  lateralis;  VPL,  ventralis  postcrolateralis;  VP, 
ventralis  posterior;  VPM,  ventralis  posteromedialis;  VTT,  ventral  tegmental  nucleus  of  Tsai;  and 
^I,  zona  incerta.   [From  Kerr  et  al.  (47).] 


particular  portions  of  the  reticular  pathways  may  con- 
vey signals  essential  to  pain  perception. 

It  is  clearly  established  that,  whatever  may  be  con- 
tributed by  upward-streaming  sensory-evoked  im- 
pulses, the  central  nervous  system  possesses  an  im- 
portant downstream  sensory  control  mechanism 
which  also  undoubtedly  contributes  to  the  perceptual 


content.  The  ner\ous  system  possesses  some  mecha- 
nism whereby  the  amplitude  of  sensory-evoked  re- 
sponses, and  hence  the  number  or  synchrony  of  units 
responding,  can  be  greatly  modified.  This  mechanism 
exerts  an  effect  within  each  of  the  classical  sensory 
pathways,  altering  the  initiation  of  impulses  or  their 
transmission  through  the  entire  succession  of  sensory 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY   TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


757 


synapses.  There  is  good  e\idence,  too,  that  this 
mechanism  is  discharged  by  way  of  the  brain-stem 
reticular  formation;  the  action  might  originate  there 
or  perhaps  elsewhere  within  the  cerebral  and  cerebel- 
lar hemispheres,  but  it  undoubtedly  funnels  through 
and  may  be  significantly  modified  by  the  reticular 
formation.  The  end  effect  of  this  mechanism  may  be 
facilitatory  or  inhibitory,  but  in  many  central  relays 
it  appears  to  be  predominantly  inhibitory.  The 
mechanism  clearly  depends  upon  an  active  process; 
its  effect  can  be  interrupted  by  transection  of  the 
neuraxis  above  the  level  of  testing,  by  deep  anesthesia 
and,  more  specifically,  by  the  placement  of  a  lesion 
in  the  central  core  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  forma- 
tion. The  dynamic  operation  of  this  mechanism  ap- 
pears to  be  responsible  during  wakefulness  for 
fluctuations  in  the  amplitude  of  sensory-evoked 
responses. 

Beha\ioral  studies,  too,  indicate  that  this  mecha- 


FiG.  14.  Evoked  potentials  following  tooth  pulp  stimulation 
recorded  from  three  loci  at  the  same  mesencephalic  level.  ^tI. 
Recorded  from  the  portion  of  the  central  tegmental  fasciculus 
ascending  alongside  the  periaqueductal  grey.  B.  Recorded  from 
the  pathway  within  the  intermediolateral  portion  of  the  central 
grey.  C,  Small  early  response  followed,  after  a  long  latency,  by 
a  second  longer  discharge,  recorded  from  the  region  of  the 
decussated  brachium  conjuncti\-um.  Note  differences  in  the 
time  scale.  Note  also  that,  although  all  three  of  these  individual 
loci  may  be  considered  parts  subsumed  within  the  general 
regional  designation  of  the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  and 
each  is  a  bilaterally  represented  pathway,  they  are  nonetheless 
distinguished  from  one  another  by  differences  in  latency,  am- 
plitude and  duration  of  response.  [From  Kerr  el  al.  (47).] 


nism  plays  a  dynamic  role  during  wakefulness.  Here 
its  operational  effect  is  usually  a  reduction  of  sensory 
signals,  an  effect  that  is  active  in  inverse  relation  to 
the  degree  of  attention  or  interest  enlisted  by  that 
particular  stimulus.  The  mechanism  seems  to  be  less 
active  (to  inhibit  less)  when  a  stimulus  is  novel  or 
when  a  stimulus  is  given  special  significance,  as  by  its 
association  with  an  important  unconditioned  stimu- 
lus. The  mechanism  appears  to  be  more  active  (to 
inhibit  more)  in  relation  to  signals  arising  from  stimuli 
to  which  habituation  has  been  developed  and  other 
stimuli,  even  though  not  rendered  ineffective  by 
habituation,  from  which  attention  has  been  with- 
drawn. 

Briefly,  this  sensory  control  mechanism  appears  to 
provide  the  perceptual  processes  with  an  active  or- 
ganizing principle,  including  an  element  of  purpose, 
which  tends  to  select  and  modify  sensory  messages 
within  the  earliest  stages  of  their  trajectory.  If  overt 
behavior  may  be  assumed  to  provide  a  cogent  index 
for  the  interpretation  of  telos,  then  this  sensory  control 
mechanism  is  designed  to  diminish  the  engagement 
of  higher  centers  with  those  signals  that  have  the  least 
significance  to  the  individual. 

A  mechanism  operating  in  this  way  requires  that 
incoming  signals  be  identified  and  given  significance. 
How  might  this  identification  and  attachment  of  value 
come  about?  Only  partial  answers  can  be  provided  at 
this  time.  Continuous  electrographic  recordings  from 
multiple  sites  indicate  that,  when  a  behaving  animal 
encounters  a  new  situation,  at  first  a  very  large  terri- 
tory of  the  brain  is  drawn  into  a  novel  activity.  As  the 
experience  is  repeated  many  times,  there  develops  a 
significant  economy  in  terms  of  the  extent  of  brain 
involvement.  Perhaps  recognizable  signals  can  eventu- 
ally be  reduced  to  a  quite  small  number  of  impulses, 
representing  minuscule  abstractions  of  reality.  Perhaps 
recognizable  identity  can  be  established  e\en  before 
the  sensory-evoked  impulses  have  time  to  ascend  ail 
the  way  to  cortex  and  back.  Something  of  a  parallel 
sort  appears  to  take  place  within  motor  circuits  as  one 
proceeds  from  the  execution  of  a  complex  novel  move- 
ment to  that  same  movement  when  it  is  established 
as  an  ingrained  motor  habit.  There  is  evidently  an 
analogous  economization  and  automatization  of 
neuronal  activity  in  relation  to  the  habituated  act  as 
finally  executed. 

The  attachment  of  value  to  such  identified  signals 
could  presumably  come  about  quite  naturally  through 
the  activation,  pari  passu,  of  certain  portions  of  the 
brain's  primary  reinforcement  systems  (see  Chapter 
LXII  l)y  Stellar  in  this  work).  .\  number  of  the  struc- 


758 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


FIG.  15.  Effect  of  nitrous  oxide  on  long  latency  bilateral  reticular  formation  responses  to  tooth 
pulp  stimulation.  Although  the  lemniscal  response  to  tooth  pulp  stimulation  is  not  perceptibly 
aflfected  by  nitrous  oxide-oxygen  inhalation,  responses  elicited  within  both  ipsilateral  and  contra- 
lateral reticular  formation  practically  disappear  after  5  min.  of  such  inhalation.  Recovery  is  nearly 
complete  15  min.  later.  [From  Haugen  &  Melzack  (34).] 


tures  implicated  in  both  positive  and  negative  rein- 
forcement undoubtedly  participate  in  the  central  inte- 
gration of  both  sensory  and  motor  mechanisms.  These 
structures  are  anatomically  linked  with  the  extra- 
lemniscal,  diffusely  projecting  and  extrapyramidal 
systems  as  well  as  the  phylogenetically  older  parts  of 
the  cortex  and  brain  stem. 

There  has  long  been  a  quest  to  know  how  nerve 
signals  might  be  'read'  and  how  they  might  be  gi\en 
'value.'  We  are  now  certainly  closer  to  knowing  where 
such  events  take  place  even  though  the  how  is  still 
unanswered.  Clearly  the  cortex  is  not  the  first  step  in 
sensory  integration.  During  wakefulness  sensory  inte- 
gration is  taking  place  continuously  and  dynamically, 
beginning  with  the  farthest  afferent  outposts.  This 
involves  an  erosion  of  information  that  originally 
started  into  the  nervous  system  and  an  intrusion  of 
influences  which  are  based  upon  the  animal's  previous 
experience  as  well  as  its  momentary  disposal  of  atten- 
tion. This  implies  that  there  is  a  reduction  and  distor- 
tion of  sensor\-cvoked  signals  from  the  acttial  nature 
of  the  stimulating  world.  Perhaps  'value'  is  likewise 
inserted  into  the  complex  at  these  early  stages  of 
sensory  integration.  Certainly  significance  to  the 
organism  appears  to  be  a  guiding  principle  with 
respect  to  the  operation  of  sensory  control  mecha- 
nisms, hence  a  survival  of  incoming  impulses  in  the 


unanesthetized   brain  would  appear  to   he  jnima  Jacie 
evidence  of  their  significance. 

In  order  to  bridge  the  gap  between  neurophysi- 
ology and  psychology,  it  is  necessary  somehow  to 
determine  the  neuronal  mechanisms  underlving  be- 
havior. A  principal  difliculty  appears  with  the  at- 
tempt to  interpret  the  fimction  of  the  whole  out  of  its 
parts.  Whereas  the  beha\  ior  of  separate  parts  could 
be  made  out  from  an  analysis  of  the  interaction  of  one 
part  with  another,  these  parts  will  not  add  together 
in  any  simple  fashion  to  account  for  the  beha\ior  of 
the  whole.  There  ha\e  been  recent  attempts  to  char- 
acterize the  differences  between  linear  cause-and- 
effect  relationships  and  the  more  inxolved  dynamics 
of  a  large  number  of  mutually  interdependent  systems 
in  simultaneous  action  (55).  The  authors  use  the  term 
'trans-action'  to  signify  the  latter  kinds  of  operations 
and  to  contrast  them  with  more  limited  'interaction' 
systems.  .Attempts  ha\e  been  made  to  interpret  some 
of  the  transactional  mechanisms  invoked  in  \isceral 
sensation  and  emotional  expression  (54). 


SUMM.XRY 

Recent  experimental  evidence,  drawn  largely  from 
the  studv  of  animals  without  central  anesthesia,  indi- 
cates that  the  nersous  .system  is  much  more  plastic  in 


CENTRAL    CONTROL    OF    RECEPTORS    AND    SENSORY    TRANSMISSION    SYSTEMS 


759 


its  action  than  previously  believed.  XN'hat  may  be 
taken  for  sensory  pathways,  because  they  convey 
sensory-evoked  signals  in  a  centripetal  direction,  turn 
out  to  be  more  variable  and  more  widespread  in  their 
transmission  of  impulses  in  the  waking  state.  The  in- 
creased variability  seems  to  be  due  to  active  inter- 
ference by  a  centrifugal  mechanism.  The  widespread 
distribution  of  sensory-evoked  impulses  allows  a  more 
elaborate  intermingling  of  sensory  with  other  signals 
throughout  various  parts  of  the  brain.  Experimental 
evidence  favors  a  lessening  of  our  ccjnceptual  isolation 
of  sensory  from  motor  and  other  central  mechanisms. 
The  nervous  system  appears  to  be  made  up  less  of 
independent  linear  pathways  than  of  mutually  inter- 
dependent loop  circuits  which  stitch  together  the 
\arious  parts  of  the  brain  into  a  functional  whole. 


Along  ascending  as  well  as  descending  projections, 
the  brain-stem  reticular  formation  and  the  cerebral 
and  cerebellar  systems  linked  closely  with  it  seem  to 
modulate  impulse  traffic  in  a  continuous  action  that 
modifies  the  composition  of  perceptive  as  well  as  pro- 
jective neural  patterns.  The  losses  and  distortions  of 
signals  brought  about  by  this  mechanism  favor  the 
conclusion  that  some  teleological  mechanism  is  at 
work;  this  appears  to  be  designed  to  diminish  the  in- 
volvement of  higher  centers  with  signals  that  have 
little  immediate  significance  for  the  animal.  Thus, 
sensory  signals  appear  to  be  subject  not  only  to  error, 
in  the  sense  projected  by  Descartes,  but  also  to  some 
purposive  central  control.  A  further  examination  of 
these  mechanisms  will  help  us  to  understand  many 
problems  of  absorbing  interest  in  neurology,  psychia- 
try, psychology  and  philosophy. 


REFERENCES 


1.  Adey,  W.  R.,  N.  C.  R.  Merrillees  and  S.  Sunderland.  24. 
Brain  79:  414,   1956.  25. 

2.  Adey,  W.  R.,  J.   P.   Seoundo  and    R.   B.   Livingston.  J.  26. 
Neurophysiol.   20;   i,    1957-  27. 

3.  Adey,  W.  R.,  S.  Sunderland  and  C.  W.  Dunlop.  Elrrtrn- 
encephalog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  9:  309,  1957.  28. 

4.  Adrian,  E.  D.  The  Basis  of  Sensation;  the  Action  of  the  .Sense 
Organs.  London:  Christophers,   1928.  29. 

5.  Adrian,  E.  D.  J.  Physiol.   100:   159,   1941.  30. 

6.  Adrian,  E.  D.  Brain  66:  289,  1943. 

7.  Adrian,  E.  D.  The  Physical  Background  of  Perception.  Oxford:  31. 
Clarendon  Press,  1947. 

8.  Amassian,  V.   E.  and  R.   V.  DeVito.  J.  .Neurophysiol.    17:  32. 

575.  1954-  33- 

9.  Bremer,  F.   and  C.  Terzuolo.  Arch,  intemat.  physiol.    62: 

157.  '954-  34- 

10.  Brouwer,  B.  J.  Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.  77:  621,  1933. 

11.  BusER,    P.    AND   p.   Borenstein.    Electtoencephalog.  &    Clin.  35. 
.Neurophysiol.  Suppl.  6:  89,    1957.  36. 

12.  Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.   16:   117,   1953. 
Chang,  H.-T.  J.  Neurophysiol.  16:  133,   1953.  37. 
Dempsey,   E.   W.,   R.   S.   Morison   and   B.   R.   Morison. 

Am.  J.  Physiol.   131:  718,   1941.  38. 

15.  DoDT,  }^.  J.  Neurophysiol.   19:301,    1956. 

16.  Eldred,  E.  and  B.  Fujimori.  In:  Reticular  Formation  of  the  39. 
Brain,  edited  by  H.  H.  Jasper  et  al.  Boston :  Little,  1 958. 

17.  Eldred,   E.,   R.   Granit  and   P.   A.   Merton.   J.   Physiol.  40. 
122:  498,  1953.  41. 

18.  EvARTS,  E.  V.  AND  H.  W.  Magoun.  Science  125:  1 147,  1957. 

19.  Fischgold,  H.  and  H.  Gastaut  (editors).  Eleclroencephalog.  42. 
&  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  SuppL  6,    1957. 

20.  Forbes,  A-   and    B.   R.   Morison.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  2:   112,  43. 

1939- 

21.  French,  J.  D.,  R.  Hernandez-Peon  and  R.  B.   Livings-  44. 
TON.  J.  Neurophysiol.   18:  74,   1955. 

22.  French,  J.  D.  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  A.M. A.  Arch.  .Neurol.  45. 
&  Psychiat.  68:  591,   1952. 

23.  French,  J.  D.,  M.  Verzeano  AND  H.  W.  Magoun.  .4. ;l/..l.  46. 
Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  69:  519,   1953. 


■3- 
14. 


FusTER,  J.  M.  Fed.  Proc.   16:  43,   1957. 

Galambos,  R.  Physiol.  Rev.  34:  497,    1954. 

Galambos,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.   19:  424,   1956. 

Galambos,  R.,  G.  Sheatz  and  V.  G.  Vernier.  Science  123: 

376,  1956. 

Granit,    R.    Receptors   and  Sensory   Perception.    New   Haven: 

Yale  Univ.  Press,  1955. 

Granit,  R.  J.  Neurophysiol.   18:  388,   1955. 

Granit,  R.  and  B.  R.  Kaada.  Acta  physiol.  scandinav.  27: 

130,  1952. 

Hagbarth,  K.-E.  and  D.  I    B.  Kerr.  J.  Neurophysiol.  17: 

^95.  1954- 

Hampson,  J.  L.  J.  .Neurophysiol.   12:  37,   1949. 

Hanbery,   J.    AND    H.   Jasper.    J.   Neurophysiol.    16:   252, 

1953- 

Haugen,  F.   p.   and  R.   Melzack.   Anesthesiology    18:   183, 

1957-  _ 

Hernandez-Peon,  R.  Acta  neurol.  latinoam.   i  :  256,   1955. 
Hernandez-Peon,    R.    .■^nd    K.-E.    Hagbarth.   J.   .Neuro- 
physiol. 18:  44,   1955. 
Hernandez-Peon,   R.    and   H.   Scherrer.   Fed.   Proc.    14: 

71.  1955- 

Hernandez-Peon,    R.,    H.    Scherrer    and    M.   Jouvet. 

Science   123:  331,   1956. 

Hernandez-Peon,   R.,   H.   Scherrer  and   M.   Velasco. 

Acta  neurol.  latinoam.  2:  8,    1956. 

Hunt,  C.  C.  and  S.  W.  Kuffler.  J.  Physiol.  113:  283,  1951. 

Jasper,  H.,  C.   Ajmone-Marsan  and  J.   Stole.  A.Af.A. 

Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psychiat.  67:  155,   1952. 

Jasper,   H.   H.   and  J.   Droogleever-Fortuyn    .4.   Res. 

Nerv.  &  Menl.  Dis.,  Proc.  26:  272,   1947. 

Jasper,  H.,  R.  Naquet  and  E.  E.  King.  Electroenccf/ialog. 

&  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  7:  99,   1955. 

JouvET,   M.    and  J.-E.    Desmedt.    Compt.    rend.   Acad,   sc, 

Paris  243:   1916,   1956. 

Jouvet,  M.  and  R.  Hernandez-Pe6n.  Electroencephalog.  & 

Clin.  Neurophysiol.  Suppl.  6:  39,   1957. 

Kerr,  D.  L  B.  and  K.-E.  Hagbarth.  J.  Neurophysiol.  18: 

362,  1955- 


760 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


47.  Kerr,  D.  I.  B.,  !•'.  P.  Haugen  and  R.  Melzack.  Am,  J. 
Physiol.   183:  253,   1955. 

48.  King,   E.   E.,  R.   NA(iUET  and  H.  W.   Magoun.  J.   Phar- 
macol. &  Exper.   Therap.   119:  48,    1957- 

49.  KuFFLER,  S.  W.  AND  C.  EvzAGUiRRE.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.   39: 

155.  >955- 
■jo.   KuFFLER,  S.  W.  AND  C.  C.  HuNT.  A.  Res.  jVerv.  &  Menl. 

Dis.,  Proc.  30:  24,   1952. 

51.  KuFFLER,  S.   \V.,  C.  C.   Hunt  and  J.   P.   Quilliam.  J. 
Xeurophysiol.  14:  29,  1951. 

52.  Leksell,  L.  Ada  physiol.  scandinav.  Suppl.  31,   10:   1,   1945. 

53.  Lindslev,  D.  B.  :  In :  Reticular  Formation  of  the  Brain,  edited 
by  H.  H.  Jasper  el  al.  Boston:  Little,  1958. 

54.  Livingston,  R.  B.  and  F.  G.  Worden.  Stanford  .\l.  Bull.  13: 

194.  1955- 

55.  Livingston,  W.  K.,  F.  P.  Haugen  and  J.  M.  Brookhart. 

Neurology  4;  485,  1954. 

56.  Loewenstein,  W.  R.  J.  Physiol.   132:  40,   1956. 

57.  Machne,  X.,   L   Calma  and  H.   VV.   Magoun.  J.  .Keuro- 
physiol.   18:  547,  1955. 

58.  Magoun,  H.  W.  Physiol.  Rev.  30:  459,   1950. 

59.  Marshall,  W.  H.,  C.  N.  Woolsey  and  P.  Bard.  Science 

85:  388,  1937- 

60.  McKinlev,  \V.   a.   and  H.   VV.   Magoun.  .im.  J.  Physiol. 

137;  217.  ■942- 

61.  MoLLiCA,    A.,    G.  MoRUZZi  and  R.  Nacjuet.    Electroenceph- 
alog.  &  Clin.  Neurophysiol.  5:  571,   1953. 

62.  MoRisoN,  R.   S.,   E.   W.   Dempsev   and  B.   R.   Morison. 
Am.  J.  Physiol.   131:  732,   1941. 

63.  Moruzzi,    S.    and    H.    W.    Magoun.    Electroencephalog.   & 
Clin.  Neurophysiol.    I:  455,    1949. 

64.  Pavlov,   L   P.   Lectures  on  Condiliuned  Reflexes.   New  YorP; : 
Internal.  Pub.,   1928. 


65.  R.^MtJN  V  C.\J.'\L,  .S.  Iliittilogie  du  Systerne  Nerveux  de  i Homrnc 
et  des  Vertebres  (reprinted  from  original  1 909- 1 1  ed.). 
Madrid:  Consejo  Superior  de  Investigaciones  Cientificas, 

'95^-55- 

66.  Ramon  -i-  C.'\jal,  S.  Studies  on  the  Cerebral  Cortex  [Limbic 
.Structures],  translated  by  L.  M.  Kraft.  Chicago:  Yr.  Bk. 
Pub.,  1955. 

67.  R.^SMUSSEN,  G.   L.    7-  Cump.  .\eurot.  84:   141,    1946. 

68.  R.ASMUSSEN,  G.  L.   J.  Comp.  Neurol.  99:  61,   1953. 

69.  Rose,  J.  E.  and  V.  B.  Mountgastle.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 
Hosp.  94:  238,  1954. 

70.  Sgheibel,  M.,  a.  Sgheibel,  A.  Mollica  and  G.  Mo- 
ruzzi. J.  .Neurophysiol.   18:  309,   1955. 

71.  ScHERRER,   H.    AND   R.   Hernandez-Peon.   Fed.    Proc.    14: 

13^.  '955- 

72.  Segundo,  J.  P.,  R.  Arana  and  J.  D.  French.  J.  Neuro- 
surg.    12  :  601,   1955. 

73.  Segundo,  J.  P.,  R.  Naquet  and  P.  Buser.  J.  .Neurophysiol. 
18:  236,  1955. 

74.  Snider,   R.   S.   A..\1..\.  Arch.  .Neurol.  £?   Psychiat.  64:    196, 

I950- 

75.  Snider,   R.   S.   and   \.  Stowell.  J.  Neurophysiol.   7:  331, 

1944- 

76.  Starzl,  T.   E.   and  H.   \V.   Magoun.  J.  Neurophysiol.    14: 

■33.  '95'- 

77.  Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  W.  Taylor  and  H.  VV.  M.agoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.    14:  461,    1951. 

78.  Starzl,  T.  E.,  C.  \V.  Taylor  and  H.  W.  Magoun.  J. 
.Neurophysiol.  1 4 :  479,   1 95 1  ■ 

79.  Starzl,  T.   E.  and  D.  G.  VVhitlogk.  J.  Neurophysiol.   15: 

449.  '95'^- 

80.  Therman,  p.  O.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:   153,   1941. 

8 1 .  von  Euler,  C.  and  G.  Rigci.  J.  .Neurophysiol.  21:  231, 1958. 


INDEX 


Index 


ACA  ratio:  sec  Accommodation 
Acceleration    stimuli:   see    Equilibrium 
Accommodation,  654-656,  660-664 

ACA  ratio 

definition,  664 

accommodative  convergence  and,  664 

age  and,  664 

convergence  and,  662 

definition,  656 

innervation  controlling,  662 

lens  and,  660 

limiting  factor,  664 

mechanism,  660 

night  myopia  and,  664 

phoria  and,  664 

pupils  and,  662 

refracting  mechanism  and,  655 

response  to  blur,  663 

.Scheincr  principle  and,  659 

sky  myopia  and,  664 

threshold  level  and,  97 

with  fixed  stimulus,  660 

zero  level,  659 
Accommodative    convergence:    see    Con- 
vergence 
Acetylcholine 

see  also  Cholinergic  transmitter;  Trans- 
mitter substances;  Cvu'arc;  Neuro- 
muscular transmission ;  Parasym- 
pathin 

arousal  and,  1  79 

as  pain  excitant,  479 

as  transmitter  substance,  1 39,  1 55, 
166,  179,  200,  230 

characteristics,  231 

competition  with  curare,  210 

depolarization  and,  210 

electrically  inexcitable  membrane  and, 

■55 
intermittent  release,  207 
mode  of  action,  2 1  o 
mollusc  muscle  and,  248 
sodium  and,  210 
substances  blocking,  139 
thermoreceptors  and,  455 


Action  potential 

see  also  Evoked  potential 

abolition,  100 

absolute  refractory  period  and,  308 

activity  and,  378 

auditory  nerve,  575 

axoplasm,    longitudinal    current    and, 
103 

current  theories,  1 1  7 

excitability  and,  99 

giant  axon,  84 

junctional  activity  and,  205 

membrane 
definition,  84 

membrane  potential,  100 
time  course  and,  103 

monophasic,  77 

muscle  in  invertebrates,  242 

Na  theory  and,  1 18 

non-linear  phenomena,  95 

polarizing  current  and,  1 1 2 

prolonged  abolition,  loi 

retina,  617 

temporal   relation    to    membrane   cur- 
rent, 104 
Adaptation 

see  also  Photic  adaptation;  Scoptic 
adaptation 

definition,  125 

double  pain  and,  473 

in  pain  receptors  and  fibers,  468,  473 

in  thermal  receptors,  456 

retinal  receptive  fields  and,  705 

taste  sense  and,  524 

to  touch-pressure,  403 

vestibular  mechanism  and,  555 
Adrenaline :  see  Epinephrine 
Adrenergic  transmitter,  218-230 

see  also  Epinephrine;  Norepinephrine; 
Dopamine;  Isopropylnorcpincph- 
rine;  Catechol  amines;  Transmitter 
substances 

biosynthesis  of,  220-222 

characteristics,  218,  220 

dopamine  as,  229 

763 


epinephrine  as,  140,  179,  218,  229 

exhaustibility,  227 

identification  of,  218-220 

iproniazide  and,  224 

isopropylnorepinephrine  as,  229 

other  than  norepinephrine,  228 

release,  222-227 

removal  of,  227,  228 

remote  effects,  225 

stimulus  frequency  and,  222 

storage,  221 
After-discharge 

classification,  312 

cortical,  thalamic  connections,  307 

decamethoniuni  iodide,  306 

definition,  305 

isolated  cortex,  306 

medullary  pyramid,  306 

rhythmic,  307 

specific   neuronal   circuits    and,    307 

types,  305 
After -potential 

definition,  1 15 

fiber  type  and,  1 15 

membrane  resistance  and,  1 1 5 

negative,  1 15 
Age 

electroencephalogram,  human 
beta  activity,  297 
delta  activity,  296 
theta  activity,  297 

lens  structure  and,  662 

taste  bud  distribution  and,  508 
Alcohol  dehydrogenase 

visual  pigments  and,  673 
Alkaline  phosphatase 

in  olfactory  mucosa,  539 
All-or-none  response,  64,  76,  79 

conversion  to  graded,  168 

lack  in  protozoa,  370 
Alteration  of  response  theory:  see  Vision 
Aluminum  cream 

lesions  due  to,  351 


764 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Ametropia 

correction,  655 

definition,  655 
Amino  acids 

action  on  synapses,  i  77 
7-Aminobutyric  acid 

action  on  synapses,  177 

inhibitory  synapses  and,  162 

mode  of  action,  1 78 
Amygdala 

lesions  in,  351 

stimulation  of,  351 
Analeptics 

electrical  discharge  and,  340 
Anesthesia 

auditory   cortical   activity   and,    599 

brain  excitability  and,  308 

hyperpathia  and,  477 

medial  lemniscal  system  and,  406 

olfactory  bulb  activity  and,  541,  542 

reticular  formation  inhibition  and,  745 

sensory  pathways  and,  752,  755 

sensory  responses  and,  747 

sensory  somatic  stimuli  and,  423 
Anesthetics 

central    neuron    escitability    and,    389 
Angular  acceleration 

threshold  for  perception  of,  554 
Annelids 

see  also   Invertebrates 

eye,  camera  style,  638 
Anoxia 

cochlear  microphonic  and,  577 

cochlear  summating  potential  and,  578 

convulsions 

reticular  formation  and,  339 
theory,  344 

d.c.  potential  and,  318 

EEG  and,  339 

pre-   and  postsynaptic   potentials   and, 
302 
Aphakia 

definition,  656 

ultraviolet  light  and,  666 
Appetitive  behavior 

see  also  Behavior;  Self-selection  studies; 
Conditioned  reflex 

taste  and,  527 
Aqueous  humor 

index,  656 
Arterial  pressure 

brain  excitability  and,  308 
Arteries 

volume  change  and  pain,  463 
Arthropods 

see  also   In\ertebrates;  Insects;  Crusta- 
ceans 

camera  style  eyes  in,  639 

compound  eye 

polarization  plane  of  light  and,  636 

cone  cells  in  eyes,  633 

muscle  innervation,  240 


neuromuscular  transmission  in,  240 

ocelli  in,  630 

polarized  light  and,  636 
.Asphyxia 

blocking  of  nerve  fibers  and,  471,  472 

d.c.  potential  and,  318 
Atropine 

mode  of  action,  233 
Attention 

neural  mechanism  of,  753-759 

reticular  formation  and,  367 
Audition,  565-612 

see  also  parts  of  the  ear  and  related  terms 

acuity 

auditory  cortex  and,  596 
pure  tone  threshold  and,  596 

central  mechanisms  of,  585-612 

decortication  and,  595 

descending  fibers  and,  591 

interaction  with  visual  impulses,  3 1 1 

invertebrate,  381,  382 

range  of,  565 

temporal  information 
transmission  of,  583 

theory  of,  58 1  -584 
Auditory  conditioning 

experimental  production,  753 
Auditory  habituation,  752,  753 
Auditory  cortex,  591-609 

see  also  Central  auditory  function 

anatomical  area,  592 

cochlear  representation  in,  594,  606 

evoked  potentials 
distraction  and,  754 

hearing  acuity  and,  596 

integrative  function,  598 

localization  of  sound  in  space,  598 

medial  geniculate  and,  598 

periodic  excitability  change,  309 

postexcitatory  depression  and,  309 

primary  and  secondary,  594 

refractory  period,  308 

species  differences  in,  592 

thalamic  nuclei  and,  598 

third  area,  596 

tonal  pattern  discrimination,  597 

tonotopic  projection  in,  603 

topological  projection  in,  603 
.\uditory  habituation 

experimental  production,  752 
Auditory  localization 

transmission  of,  583 
Auditory  nerve,  579-581 

action  potentials,  575 

central  control,  744 

efferent  fibers  in,  744 

efferent  inhibition,  580 

frequency  response,  580 

impulses  in,  579 

latency  in,  579 

parallel     ascending     and     descending 
pathways,  750 


recruitment  and,  31 1 

single  fiber  activity  in,  579 

volley  principle  in,  579 
Auditory  reception 

in  man  and  insects,  374 
Autonomic  nervous  system 

see  also  Parasympathetic  nerves;   Sym- 
pathetic nervous  system 

neuroeflTector  transmission  in,   215-235 

pain  and,  480-483 
Axon 

see  also  Nerve  fibers 

function,  59 

membrane  as  condenser,  85 

squid,  as  cable,  85 
Axoplasm 

longitudinal  current  in,  103 

Barbiturates 

brain  excitability  and,  308 
corticopetal  system  and,  389 
refractory  period  and,  308 
synaptic  block  and,  301 
Basilar  membrane :  see  Cochlea 
Bechterew's  nucleus 

equilibrium  and,  558 
Behavior 

see  also  Self-selection  studies,  Appetiti\e 

behavior;  Conditioned  reflex 
attention  and,  752-755 
neuronal  mechanisms  of,  754-759 
olfaction  and,  547 
response  to  light,  728 

cells  with  photoreceptors,  624 
cells  without  photoreceptors,  623 
reticular  formation  and,  755 
taste  and,  527 
Binaural  stimulation 

definition,  556 
Bitter  taste 

modification  by  experience,  529 
substances  giving,  520 
Body  temperature 

EEG,  alpha  activity  and,  296 
Brachium    of   inferior    coUiculus:    see    In- 
ferior quadrigeminal  brachium 
Bradykinin 

as  pain  excitant,  479 
Brain 

see  also  Central  nervous  system;  Spinal 

cord;  individual   parts   of  the   brain 

electrical  activity  of,  255-258,  279-297, 

'^99-3'^.   3 '5-360,   716-727 
evoked  potentials  of,  299-312 
excitability 

afferent   impulse   inflow   and,   310 
factors  affecting,  308 
moisture 

excitability  and,  308 
rliythmic  activity 
generation  of,  280 


INDEX 


76: 


harmonic  and  relaxation  oscillators, 
281 
oscillators,  281 

responsiveness  to  stimuli,  283 
simple  harmonic  motion,  280 
spontaneous,  279,  282,  283 
Brain  potentials,  255-258,  279-297,  299- 
312,315-360,716-727 
characteristics 

functional  significance,  256 
nature,  255 

neuron  characteristics  affecting,  257 
rhythmicity  of,  258 
Brain  rhythms :  see  Electroencephalogram 
Brain   stem  reticular   formation:  see   Re- 
ticular formation 
Brightness 

definition  of,  715 
measurement  of,  729 
nature  of,  729 
Brightness  vision,  729,  732-735,  737 
see  also  Vision 
contrast 

definition  of,  715 
decortication  and,  728 
enhancement 

description  of,  732 
neurophysiological  explanation,  732 
Buffer  solutions 
taste  of,  514 
Bulbar   relays:   see   Medullary   oblongata 
Burning  pain:  see  Pain 

Calcium 

end  plate  potential  and,  208 
Caloric  stimulation 

of  endolymph,  556 
Carbon  dioxide 

thermoreceptors  and,  455 
Catechol  amines 

see  also  Norepinephrine;  Epinephrine; 
Dopamine;  Isopropylnorcpineph- 
rine;  Adrenergic  transmitter;  Trans- 
mitter substances 

remote  effects  of,  225 

urinary  excretion  of,  225 
Caudate  nucleus 

convulsion  inhibition  by,  344 
Central  auditory  function,  585-612 

see  also  Auditory  cortex 

dispersion  of  excitation,  61  i 

frequency  specificity,  607 

functional  requirements,  587 

inhibition,  61 1 

lateral  lemniscus  and  nucleus,  589 

laterality  of  projection,  609 

loudness  and,  609 

recurrent  pathway,  61  I 
Central     excitatory     state:     see     Central 

nervous  system 
Central     inhibitory     state:     see     Central 
nervous  system 


Central  ner\  ous  system 
axons 

potentials  from,  268 
control  of  triad  response,  663 
excitatory  state 

depolarizing  p.s.p.s  and,  164 
field  currents  in,  igi 
inhibitory  state 

hyperpolarizing  p.s.p.'s  and,  164 
microelectrode  studies  in,  262 
micropipette  techniques  in,  263 
motoneurons 

recording  from,  271 
neurons 

excitability  states,  389 

for  kinesthesis,  414 
primary  sensory  fibers 

recording  from,  270 
single  fiber  isolation,  262 
single  units 

activity,  261-276 

identification,  267 
Cephalopods 

see  also  Invertebrates;  Molluscs 
pupil  in,  637 
Cerebellum 

auditory  pathway  and,  590,  591 
Cerebral  cortex 

ablation,  pain  and,  493 
afferent  impulse  interaction,  310 
anemia 

d.c.  potential  and,  318 
auditory  projection  system  and,  591 
corticifugal  sensory  control,  749-752 
d.c.  potentials  in,  315-327 
excitability  of,  310,  311 
functional    unity    of  vertical    columns, 

415 
interaction  systems  in,  310,  748 
isolated 

after-discharges,  306 
lesions  in  epilepsy,  349 
pain  and,  492-498 
piriform  stimulation,  351 
postcentral  homologue 

patterns,  402 
reticular  control  of  afferents  to,  747-749 
somesthetic  area 

electrical  stimulation  of,  436 
strychnine  and,  340 
taste  representation  in,  510 
thermosensitive    units   in,  436 
visual  mechanisms  in,  719-727 
Chemical  energy 

receptor  excitation  by,  124 
Chemical  stimuli 
taste  and,  510 
Chemical  transmission 

see  also  Transmitter  substances 
anatomy,  2  r6 
versus  electrical,  2 1 7 


Chemoreceptors 

invertebrate,  375,  376 

temperature  changes  and,  376 
Choline  acetylase 

characterization,  232 
Cholinergic  transmitter,  230-233 

see  also  Acetylcholine;  Transmitter 
substances.  Curare;  Neuromuscular 
transmission;  Parasympathin 

acetylcholine  as,  139,  155,  166,  179, 
200,  230 

biosynthesis,  231,  232 

characteristics,  230,  231 

mechanism  of  release,  232 

parasympathin,  233 

release  of,  232,  233 

removal  of,  233 

storage,  232 
Cholinesterase 

inhibition,  233 
Cholinesterase  inhibitors 

mode  of  action,  180 
Chromaffin  cells 

storage  of  hormones  in,  221 
Ciliary  muscle 

as  limit  to  accommodation,  664 

as  skeletal  muscle,  662 

muscle  potential,  662 
Cocaine 

blocking  of  nerve  fibers,  471 

blocking  of  sensation  by,  394 

second  pain  response  and,  471 
Cochlea 

see  also  Audition;  Ear 

as  mechanical  frequency  analyzer,  571 

basilar  membrane,  width  of,  569 

bilateral  representation  of,  610 

blood  supply,  574 

cortical  representation  of,  594,  606 

efferent  control  of,  744,  745 

excitation  of,  565-584 

generator  potential,  130 
Cochlear  ner\es 

tone  frequency  response,  604 
Cochlear  nuclei 

anatomy,  587 

efferent  fibers  from,  589 
Cochlear  potentials,  575-579 

injury  and,  577 

microphonics 
anoxia  and,  577 
characteristics,  576 

summating 

anoxia  and,  578 
characteristics,  576 
Coelenterates 

see  also   Invertebrates 

effector  structures  in,  370 

neuromuscular  transmission  in,  249 
Cold 

effect  on  endolymph,  556 


766 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Cold-blooded  animals 

thermal  receptors  in,  445 
Cold  fibers 

see  also  Thermal  fibers 

discharge,  446 

and  temperature,  447 

paradoxical  discharge,  452 

temperature  change  and,  449 
Cold  receptors 

depth  in  cat  tongue,  43JJ 
Cold  sensation:  see  Thermal  sensation 
Colliculi,  inferior 

heaving  and,  589,  590 
Color  experience 

with  non-spectral  stimuli,  738 
C'olor  vision 

definition,  716 

description  of,  738 

electrophysiology  of,  706 

in  cat,  739 

in  decorticate  animal,  728 

in  lower  animals,  706 

problems  related  to,  738 
Conditioned  reflex 

see  also  Behavior;  Self-selection  studies; 
Appetitive  behavior 

as  test  for  auditory  function,  596 
Conditioning    nerve    impulse:    my    Nerve 

impulse,  conditioning 
Conduction,  75-1 19 

see   also    Nerve    impulse;    Transmission 

external  resistance  and,  1 06 

narcosis  and,  i  14 

polarized  fiber,   i  1  1 

retinal,  696 

safety  factor,  108 

saltatory,  106 
model,  109 

velocity 

determination  of,  103 
of  nerve  fibers,  78 
Cones 

arthropod,  633 

electroretinogram  and,  699 

flicker  fusion  and,  708 

histology  of,  693 

modulators  in,  708 

visual  pigments  in,  671 
Convergence 

accommodative 

accommodation  and,  664 

fusional 

innervation  of,  663 

relation    of    accommodative    and    fu- 
sional, 663 
Convulsions,  generalized,  329-360 

see  also  Pentylenetetrazol  seizures; 
Strychnine  convulsions;  Epilepsy; 
Anoxia,  convulsions 

cortical  potentials  and,  322,   323 

cortical  theory,  333 


eclectic  theory,  333 

electrical  discharge,  338 
duration  of,  343 
propagation  of,  350 

factors  producing,  338 

inhibition,  343 

mechanism  of  discharge,  334 

neuronal  exhaustion,  343 

produced  by  analeptics,  340 

reticular  formation  and,  335,  340 

subcortical  theory,  332 

unified  concept,  338 

without  loss  of  consciousness,  339 
Cornea 

configuration,  657 

indices  of  layers,  656 

pain  and,  465 

pain  receptors  in,  466 
Crista 

anatomy  of,  551 
Critical  flicker  frequency:  see  Flicker 
Critical  potential 

frequency   of  the   discharge   and,    128, 

13-i 
Crustaceans 

see   also    Invertebrates 

muscle 

end-plate  potentials,  243 
inhibition  in,  244 
innervation,  240 
slow  and  fast  potentials,  241 

neiu'omuscular    transmission    in,    240, 
243,  244 
Curare 

see  also  Cholinergic  transmitter;  Acetyl- 
choline; Transmitter  substances; 
Neuromuscular  transmission ;  Para- 
sympathin 

arthropod  neuromuscular  transmission 
and,  244,  247 

competition    with    acetylcholine,     210 

end-plate  potential  and,  203 

transmission  and,  149 
C'litaneous  sensations 

see  also  Thermal  sensations;  Tactile 
system;  Skin  receptors;  Touch- 
pressure  system 

activity  in  fibers  of  different  size,  394 

concept  of  Head,  391 

pattern  theory,  390 

sensory  recovery  after  section,  475 

theories  of,  390,  391 
Cyanopsin 

see  also  Visual  pigments 

as  visual  pigment,  678 

photopic  sensiti\'ity  and,  684 

Dark  adaptation 

liver  disorders  and,  690 
pigment  resynthesis  and,  686 

D.C.  potentials,  315-327 
anoxia  and,  318 


asphyxia  and,  318 

convulsoid  discharge  and,  322 

cortical  anemia  and,  318 

definition,  315 

evoked  potentials  and,  319 

factors  affecting,  315 

human  scalp,  326 

origin,  326 

polarization  and,  316 

recording  of,  316 

on  conventional  EEG,  317 

recruiting  responses  and,  320 

shift 

ECG  and,  319 

spreading  depression  and,  323 

stimiUation  multisvnaptic  path  and, 
326 

strychnine  and,  321 

veratrine  spikes  and,  321 
Uecamethonium 

after-discharge  and,  306 

effect  on  synapses,  1 79 
Dcfacilitation 

definition,  157 
Deiter's  nucleus 

equilibrium  and,  558 
Dendrites 

see  also  Nerve  fibers 

activity  of,  735 

behavior  in  optic  cortex,  726 

function,  59 

potentials  from,  273 

sustained  potentials  and,  735 
Depolarization 

see  also  Postsynaptic  potentials;  Po- 
larization 

acetylcholine  and,  210 

critical,  95 

postsynaptic  potential  during,  158 

threshold,  95 
Diencephalon 

chemical  stimulation  of,  335 

electrical  stimulation  of,  334 

origin    of   generalized    discharges,    334 
Diffuse    thalamic    projection   system:   see 
Thalamus,  diffuse  projection  system 
Diphosphopyridine  nucleotide 

visual  pigments  and,  673 
Direct    current    potentials:   see    D.C.    po- 
tentials 
Dol 

as  measure  of  pain,  463 
Dopamine 

see  also  Catechol  amines.  Norepi- 
nephrine, Epinephrine;  Isopropyl- 
norepinephrine;  .Adrenergic  trans- 
mitter; Transmitter  substance 

as  adrenergic  transmitter,  229 
Dorsal  columns 

see  also  Spinal  cord 

nuclei 


INDEX 


767 


pathway  to  ventrobasal  complex,  400 
patterns  in,  398 
patterns  of  medial  lemniscal  system  in, 

397 
relays 

reticular  formation  and,  745 
Dorsal  root  reflex 

electrical  activation,  192 
DPN:   see  Diphosphopyridine   nucleotide 
Drugs 

taste  sensitivity  and,  5 1  o 

Ear 

see  also  Audition;  Cochlea 

acoustical  properties,  567-574 

anatomy,  566-575 

electrical  responses  of,  575-578 

fluids  of,  574,  575 

frequency  characteristics,  569 

receptor  excitation  in,  565-584 
Ebbecke  phenomenon  :  sec  Thermal  sensa- 
tions 
Eel  electroplaques 

electromicroscopy  of,  151 
Eighth  cranial  nerve ;  see  Auditory  nerve 
Electric  taste 

production  of,  522 
Electrical  stimulus 

afferent  discharges  and,  128 

cortical  somesthetic  area  and,  436 

double  pain  and,  474 

EEC  and,  348 

paired 

postexcitatory  depression  and,  309 

partial  epilepsy  and,  348 

through  microelectrodes,  274 

vestibular  nerve  and,  559 
Electrocardiogram 

steady  potential  and,  319 
Electroencephalogram,  255-258,  279-297 

see  also  Electroencephalogram,  human 

absence  type  petit  mal  and,  337 

alpha  acti\'ity 

visual  blocking,  735 

amygdaloid  lesions  and,  35 1 

anoxia,  339 

cortical  lesions  and,  349 

critical  flicker  frequency  and,  731 

epileptic  seizures  and,  329 

focus    and    epileptogenic    lesions,    354 

local  application  of  strychnine,  348 

localized  electrical  stimulation  and,  348 

models,  280,  281 

partial   epilepsies   and,    331,    357,    358 

seizure  in  multiple  relay  systems  and, 

359 
SP  shift  and,  319 
Electroencephalogram,  human 
see  also  Electroencephalogram 
alpha  activity,  284,  287 

activation,  292 

afferent  signals  and,  289 


blocking,  289,  735 

body  temperature  and,  296 

distribution,  286 

efferent  signals  and,  294 

identification,  287 

implanted  electrodes,  293 

LSD  25  and,  289 

origin,  294 

pain  and,  472 

psychotechnical  tests  and,  289 

synchronization  and,  292 

theta  with,  285 
beta  activity 

age  and,  296 
complexity,  287 
delta  activity 

age  and,  296 

disease  and,  296 

theta  with,  285 
theta  activity 

age  and,  296 
variation,  287 
Electrogenesis 
cellular,  154 

electrically  excitable,  154 
electrically   inexcitable,    154-156,    159 
sustained,  156 

postsynaptic  membrane  and,  156 
synaptic 

chemicals  and,  163 

drug  inactivation,  1 76 

transducer  action  and,  189 
Electromagnetic  energy 

receptor  excitation  by,  124 
Electroneurogram 

elevations  related  to  sensation  modality, 

394 
Electroretinography,  696-704,  710 

see  also  Retina 

alcohol  and,  702 

arthropod  eye  and,  635 

characteristics,  697 

clinical  use,  710 

cone,  699 

damage  to  retina  and,  700 

glaucoma  and,  702 

photopic  adaptation  and,  699 

retinal  type  and,  698 

rod,  699 

scoptic  adaptation  and,  699 

source  of  response,  701,  703 

standard  leads,  696 

stimulus  intensity  and,  706 

stray  light  in,  667 
Emmetropia 

definition,  655 
Endocochlear  potential 

characteristics,  575 

source  of,  576 
Endocrines 

pain  and,  498 


Endolymph 

composition,  574 

flow  in  semicircular  canal,  553 

movement  due  to  caloric  stimulation, 
556 
Endolymphatic      potential :      see      Endo- 
cochlear potential 
End-plate  potentials,  202-209 

calcium  and,  208 

characteristics,  203 

conditioning  nerve  impulses  and,  208 

curare  and,  203 

definition,  149,  202 

in  absence  of  action  potential,  205 

in  crustacean  muscle,  243 

inhibition  and,  244 

magnesium  and,  208 

mammalian  muscle  fiber,  206 

miniature,  207 

uncurarized  muscle  and,  204 
Ephaptic  transmission,  190-194 

see  also  Transmission ;  Synaptic  trans- 
mission 

as  model  of  synaptic  transmission,   190 

compared  to  synaptic,  149 

evolution,  194 

excitation,  190 

nerve  cords,  192 

polarized,  192 

unpolarized  junction,  192 
Epicritic  system 

criticism  of,  475 

sensory  mechanism,  391 
Epilepsies,  partial 

anatomical  lesions,  353 

characterization,  330 

diffuse,  359 

discharges 

character  of,  357 
diffuse,  358 
erratic,  353 
localized  EEG  in,  357 

mode  of  propagation  of,  356 
neuronal,  355 
requirements  for  propagation  of,  356 

distinction  of  two  varieties,  357 

EEG  changes  in,  331 

etiology,  331 

experimental,  348 

generalized  convulsions,  357 

localized,  359 

physiopathogenesis,  354 

physiopathology,  347 

predisposing  factors,  355 

rhinencephalic,  350 

secondary  generalization,  352 

subcortical  origin,  352 
Epilepsy,  329-360 

see  also  Convulsions,  generalized; 
Pentylenetetrazol  seizures;  Strych- 
nine convulsions;  Anoxia, convulsions 

clinical  picture,  329 


768 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


cortical  lesions  in,  349 
degenerative 

reticular  formation  and,  342 
EEG  changes,  329 
focus 

behavior  of  allied  centers   and,   356 
functional,  331 
organic,  331 
postdischarges 

characteristics,  349 

cortical,  propagation,  350 

transmission,  353 

zones  for,  349 
psychomotor 

characterization,  330 
Epilepsy,  grand  mal 
characterization,  330 
cortical  theory,  333 
eclectic  theory,  333 
electrical  discharge 

causes  of,  342 

mechanism  of,  334 
electrical  discharge  and,  338 
experimental  production,  332 
pentylenetetrazol  seizures  and,  341 
physiopathology,  331 
reticular  neurons  in,  342 
subcortical  theory,  332 
Epilepsy,    partial:    see    Epilepsies,  partial 
Epilepsy,  petit  mal 

absence  type,  336,  346 

EEG  in,  337 
characterization,  330 
myoclonic,  33'j,  346 

experimental  production,  336 
thalamus  and,  337 
Epinephrine 

iee     also     Catechol     amines;     Norepi- 
nephrine; Dopamine;   Isopropylnor- 

epinephrine;  Adrenergic  transmitter; 

Transmitter  substances 
acetylcholine  and,  210 
as  transmitter  substance,  140,  179,  J 18, 

229 
differentiation     from     norepinephrine, 

218 
release  in  hypoglycemia,  226 
Equilibrium 

see  also  Vestibular  mechanism 
control  in  invertebrates,  382 
senses  affecting,  549 
Evoked  potentials,  299-312 
after-effects,  319 
anatomical  studies  and,  300 
antidromically  produced,  304 
apical  dendrites,  304 
auditory  cortex  and,  599 
compared  to  spontaneous,  299 
components,  300 
cortical 

mechanism,  303 
pain  and,  494 


definition,  299 

distraction  and,  754 

electrical  signs,  304 

electrical     stimulation     of     vestibular 
nerve  and,  559 

excitability  changes  and,  308 

external  milieu  and,  302 

internal  milieu  and,  302 

latent  period,  301 

lateral  geniculate  and,  325 

olfactory  bulb  and,  541,  545 

periodic,  307 

presynaptic   differentiation   from    post- 
synaptic, 312 

repetitive  stimulation  and,  301 

sensory  localization  and,  300 

synapse  and,  304 
Excitation 

axonal,  94-100 

mechanism    of,    in    thermal    receptors, 

456 
neuronal,  273-276 
quantitative  aspects,  129 
transmission  of  energy,  1 37 
Excitatory  synapses:  see  Synapse,  excita- 
tory 
Eye,  615-759 

see  also  Vision,  Retina;  Cornea 
accommodation  of,  654-656,  660-664 
as  optical  device,  647 
axial  chromatic  aberration  in,  668 
axial  length,  x-ray  measurement,  658 
image  formation  in,  647-691 
internal  refracting  surfaces 

measurement,  657 
measurements  in,  656 
optic  axis,  657 
Purkinje  figure  in,  669 
reaction  to  stimuli,  366 
refracting  mechanism,  654-656 

accommodation  and,  655 
refracting  power,  650 
refraction  of,  654 
schematic 

exploded,  651 

Gullstrand,  648 

Helmholtz,  651 

reduced,  651 
second  nodal  point 

x-ray  location,  658 
sensitivity  to  ultraviolet,  64 1 
spectral  transmittance,  666 
spherical  aberration,  668 
static  refraction,  655 
stray  light  in,  667 
visual  pigment  in,  617 

migration  of,  640 
Eye,  camera  style 
definition,  628 
in  annelids,  638 
in  arthropods,  639 
in  molluscs,  637 


Eye,  compound 

in  arthropods,  631,  633 

polarization  plane  of  light  and,  636 
Eye,  multicellular 

photosensitivity  in,  627 
Eyespots 

composition,  627 

simple  and  compound,  627 

unicellular,  photosensitivity,  627 

Facilitation 

definition,  168 

heterosynaptic,  185 

homosynaptic,  184 

neuromuscular  junction  and,  250,  251 

of  nerve  impulse,  1 84 
Fasciculation 

explanation,  164 
Fechner"s  paradox 

brightness  contrast  and,  737 

definition,  736 
Field  currents 

central  nervous  system  and,  191 
First  somatic  cortical  field 

fimctional  organization,  415 

somesthetic  discrimination  and,  425 

tactile  and  kinesthetic  activity  and,  423 
Flicker 

definition  of,  715 

description  of,  729 

detection 

by  arthropods,  634 

EEG  driving  and,  731 

frequency 

cortical  relation,  730 

fusion 

in  cone  eyes,  708 
in  rod  eyes,  708 
Foveal  chief  ray 

definition,  653 
Foveal  vision :  see  Vision,  foveal 
Frequency  discrimination 

transmission  of,  583 
Frontal  lobectomy 

pain  responses  and,  497,  498 
Fusion 

description  of,  729 
Fusion  point 

definition  of,  730 
Fusional  convergence :  see  Convergence 

GABA ;  see  -,  -aminobutyric  acid 
Galvanic  stimulation 

of  labyrinth,  556 
Ganglia 

photosensitivity  in,  624 
Gastropods 

see  also   Invertebrates 

ocelli  in,  630 
Generator  potential 

complex  organs,  1 30 

definition,  130 


INDEX 


769 


desensitization  and,  157 
during    sustained    depolarization,     158 
receptor  development  of,  1  27 
Geniculate  body 
lateral 

vision  and,  717 
medial 

auditory  cortex  and,  598 
auditory  pathway  and,  590 
recruitment  in,  311 
Geniculate  response 

to  optic  nerve  stimulation,  724 
Grand  mal  epilepsy:  see  Epilepsy,  grand 

mal 
Gravity  receptors 
in  otoliths,  557 
Gravity  stimuli:  see  Equilibrium 
Gustatory  fibers ;  see  Taste 

Habituation 

inhibitory  efferent  pathways  and,  757 
Hair  cells :  see  Olfactory  receptors 
Hearing:  see  Audition;  Cochlea 
Heat 

double  pain  and,  474 
eflfect  on  endolymph,  556 
Heat  conduction 

skin  and,  437 
Histamine 

as  pain  excitant,  478 
Historical  development,  1-58 
concepts 

accommodation,  648 

acetylcholine,  215 

adrenergic  transmission,  221,  225 

all-or-nothing  law,  23,  24 

auditory  cortex,  600 

auditory  projection  to  cortex,  591 

central  auditory  mechanisms,  585 

cerebral  localization,  46 

cerebrospinal  fluid,  30 

chemical  transmission,  24,  215 

conditioned  reflexes,  53,  55 

correlation    of    sound    stimuli    and 
auditory  mechanism,  586 

curare,  199 

development  scientific  method,  1-4,  9 

electrical  transmission,  14,  20,  22,  23 

electroencephalography,  49,  51,  284 

epilepsy,  51,  333 

epinephrine,  215 

evoked  potentials,  49 

ganglia,  33 

image  formation  in  eye,  648 

inhibition,  36 

irritability,  !2 

medulla  oblongata,  34 

membrane  theory,  1 1 7 

motor  cortex,  47 

motor  function,  27,  28,  48 

muscle  electrophysiology,  19 

neuromuscular  junction,  24 


neuron  theory,  59,  149 
nicotine,  199 
norepinephrine,  217 
pain,  459,  460 
pain  fibers,  480 

Pfl tiger's  law  of  contraction,  1 13 
pupillary  reflex,  42 
reciprocal  innervation,  7 
reflex  activity,  25,  30,  32,  34,  36 
reflex  arc,  35,  40,  42 
reflex  excitation,  40 
reflex  inhibition,  37,  40 
refractory  period,  39 
respiratory  center,  34 
reticular  formation,  42 1 
science  of  optics,  6 1 6 
seat  of  the  soul,   2,   4,   8,   28 
semicircular  canals,  553 
sensory  function,  27,  28,  48 
spinal  cord,  25 
spinal  shock,  33 
stepping  reflex,  35 
sympathetic  trunk,  42 
sympathins,  219 
synapse,  38 
contributors 

Accademia  del  Cimento,  5 

Adrian,  24,  255 

Altenburg,  52 

Aristotle,  i 

Auburtin,  46 

Bacon,  3 

Baglivi,  9 

Ball,  616 

Bartholow,  48 

Hartley,  255 

Beck,  50,  255 

Beevor,  48 

Bell,  28,  36,  42 

Berger,  51,  255,   279,   284 

Bernard,  21,  199 

Bernstein,  23,  148 

Bichat,  28 

Bishop,  255 

Borelli,  5 

Bouillaud,  45 

Bowditch,  23,  76 

Boyle,  7 

Bemer,  255 

Breruer,  37 

Broca,  46 

Cabanis,  47 

Caldani,  47 

Cannon,  219,  225 

Caton,  49,  225 

Croone,  7 

Gushing,  49 

Cybulski,  255 

Dale,  215,  230 

Danilewsky,  52,  255 

Davis,  52 

Descartes,  5,  31 


Dieter,  26 
Dixon,  230 

du  Bois-Reymond,  22,  148 
EUiott,  24,  215 
Erianger,  24 
Ewins,  230 
Fernel,  2,  31 
Ferrier,  48 
Fischer,  52,  255 
Fleischl  von  Marxow,  255 
Flourens,  44 
Foerster,  49,  52 
Fontana,  23,  47 
Forbes,  39 
Fritsch,  47 
Galen,  i 
Gall,  43 
Galvani,  17 
Gaskell,  38 
Gasser,  24 
Gerard,  255 
Gerlach,  38 
Gibbs,  52 
Gilberd,  3 
Gilbert:  see  Gilberd 
Glisson,  !2,  32 
Goltz,  46 
Gotch,  24 
Gozzano,  255 
Hales,  32 
Hall,  34 
Haller,  1 1,  47 
Hamill,  230 
Harvey,  4 
Hecht,  616 

Helmholtz,  23,  26,  75,  616 
Hering,  37,  39 
Herringham,  30 
Hitzig,  47 
Horsley,  48 
Humboldt,  18 
Hunt,  230 
Jackson,  48,  54 
Jasper,  255 
Keen,  48 
KoUiker,  27 
Kornmiiller,  255 
Krause,  148 
Kronecker,  37 
Kiihne,  24,  148,  201,  616 
Laycock,  54 
Legallois,  34 
Lennox,  52 
Lewes,  55 
Loewi,  25,  215 
Lower,  7 
Lucas,  24 
Luciani,  45 

Magendie,  29,  42,  44,  45,  47 
Matteucci,  19 
Matthews,  255 
Mayo,  42 


770 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Mayow,  7 

Monro,  14 

Miiller,  20,  34 

Newton,  8 

Pavlov,  55 

Penfield,  49 

Pfliiger,  22 

Plato,  40 

Praudicz-Neminsky,  255 


Piochaska.  2 


7.  33 


Purkinje,  26,  261 

Remak,  26 

Rolando,  44,  47 

Rosenblueth,  219,  225 

Rosenthal,  38 

Ruffini,  30 

Schaefer,  48 

Schneider,  42 

Schwann,  26 

Sciamanna,  48 

Sechenov,  52,  54 

Sherrington,  30,  35,  36,  39,  40,  56, 
148,  149 

Soemmering,  27,  41 

Spencer,  54 

Taveau,  230 

Tiirck,  30 

Ukhtonisky,  149 

Unzer,  33 

Vesalius,  3 

von  Gerlach :  see  Gerlach 

von  Haller :  see  Haller 

von  Helmholtz ;  see  Helmholtz 

von  Humbolt:  see  Humbolt 

von  Kolliker:  see  KoUiker 

von  Soemerring :  see  Soemerring 

Waldeyer-Hartz,  27 

Waller,  26 

Walter,  52 

Wang,  255 

Wedensky,  39 

West,  36 

Whytt,  32,  41 

Willis,  7,  31,  41 

Winslow,  31 

Young,  648 
Hofmeister  series:  see  Taste  threshold 
Hot  sensation :  see  Thermal  sensations 
5-HT:  see  5-Hydro.\ytryptamine 
5-Hydroxytryptamine 
as  pain  excitant,  479 
as  transmitter  substance,  1 79 
mollusc  muscle  and,  248 
Hyperalgesia 

trauma  causing,  478 
Hyperopia 

definition,  655 
Hyperpolarization :   see    Postsynaptic    po- 
tentials, hyperpolarizing 
Hypoglycemia 

epinephrine  release  and,  226 


Hyperpathia 

anesthesia  and,  477 
Hypothermia 

pre-  and   postsynaptic   potentials   and, 
302 

Illuminance 

delinition,  647,  665 
Image  formation,  647-691 

see  also  Eye;  Vision;  Retinal  image 

astigmatism  and,  652 

lines  of  sight  and,  653 

pupil  and,  652 

pupillary  axis  and,  653 

refracting  mechanism  and,  649 

size  of  retinal  image  and,  654 
Implicit  time 

definition  of,  728 

of  flicker,  730 

relation  to  stimulus  area,  728 
Inferior  colliculus 

auditory  fibers  in,  589 
Inferior  quadrigeminal  brachium 

auditory  pathway  and,  590 
Inflammation 

pain  and,  463 
Inhibition 

afferent  in  medial  lemniscal  system,  408 

auditory  nerve  and,  580 

as  distinct  from  occlusion,  310 

central  nervous  system  and,  188 

central  paths  for,  70,  7 1 

crustacean  muscle  and,  244 

end-plate  potential  and,  244 

membrane  potential  and,  245 

muscle  membrane  and,  245 

pain  and,  499,  500 

pathways 
central,  70 
interneurons  of,  7 1 

referred  pain  and,  500 

retina  and,  706 

seizures  and,  344,  346 

sensory  neuron,  379 

sodium  ions  and,  70 

strychnine  tetanus  and,  561 

synaptic  mechanism  of,  68-70 

transmitters  for,  71-72 
Inhibitory  synapses:  see  Synapse,  inhibi- 
tory 
Injury 

hyperalgesia  and,  478 

pre-   and   postsynaptic   potentials   and, 
302 

response  of  cochlear  potentials,  577 
Injury  potential 

components,  326 

d.c.  recording  and,  316 
Insects 

see  also   Invertebrates 

binocular  vision,  635 


eye  sensitivity  to  ultraviolet,  641 

muscle 

multiterminal  innervation,  247 
slow  and  fast  contraction,  246 
units  in,  245 

neuromuscular  transmission  in,  245 

receptor  cells  in,  373 

retina  in,  631 

sense  organs  in,  373 
Integrative  activity 

synapses  and,  182 

utility  of  electrical  inexcitability,  187 
Intensity  discrimination 

transmission  of,  583 
Intensity-time  relation:  see  Strength-dura- 
tion relation 
Interneuron 

definition,  272 
Invertebrates 

see  also  Unicellular  organisms;  Pelecy- 
pods;  Multicellular  organisms;  In- 
sects; Gastropods;  Crustaceans; 
Coelenterates ;  Cephalopods;  Arthro- 
pods; Annelids;  Molluscs 

axon  conduction,  1 1  1 

chemoreceptors  in,  375 

color  sision,  640 

form  perception,  641 

hearing,  381 

mechanoceptors,  380 

muscle 

conduction,  250 
end-plate  potentials,  243 
responses,  174 

non-photic  receptors,  369 

pattern  recognition,  641 

Purkinje  shift,  640 

receptor  cells  in,  371,  375 

response  to  dynamic  stimuli,  382 

response  to  static  stimuli,  382 

sense  organs  compared  with  verte- 
brate, 374 

spectral  sensiti\ity,  640 

squid  axon  as  cable,  85 

statocysts  in,  382 

stretch  receptors 

efferent  control  of,  743 

tactile  sense,  380 

thermoreceptors,  379 

true  receptors,  371 

tympanal  organs,  382 

vibration  sense  in,  380 
lodopsin 

see  also  Visual  pigments 

bleaching  and  resynthesis,  687 

photopic  sensitivity  and,  682 
Ionic  hypothesis,  62-65,  93,  94,  118,  119 

explanation  of  properties  of  nerve 
fibers,  64 

refractory  period  and,  64 

synaptic  transmission  and,  63 


INDEX 


771 


Ionic  pump 

Na,  K  concentrations  and,  60,  62 
Iproniazide 

adrenergic  transmitter  and,  224 

removal  of  catechol  amines  and,  227 
Ischemia 

pain  and,  463,  474 

resistance  of  nerve  fibers  to,  395 

thermal  receptors  and,  442,  443,  453 
Isopropyl  isonicotinyl  hydrazine ;  s^f  Ipro- 
niazide 
Isopropylnorepinephrine 

see  also  Catechol  amines;  Norepineph- 
rine; Epinephrine;  Dopamine;  Adre- 
nergic transmitter;  Transmitter  sub- 
stance 

as  adrenergic  transmitter,  229 
Itching 

see  also  Pain 

as  related  to  pain,  498,  499 

intracisternal   injection    of  drugs    and, 

499 
intraventricular  injection  of  drugs  and, 

499 

Joint  receptors 

central  projection,  413 

discharge  patterns  of,  411 

Ruffini  type  endings,  412 
J  oints 

innervation  of,  411-415 

sensations  from,  409-415 

Kinesthesis,  388-390,  395-415 

central  neurons  for,  414 

definition,  388 

description  of,  409 

invertebrate  receptors  for,  376-379 

joint  receptors  and,  41  i 

muscle  stretch  receptors,  410 

postcentral  fields  and,  423 

sites  of  receptors,  410 
Kinesthetic  systems,  387-426 

central  classification,  396 

central  representation,  395 
Krause  end  bulbs 

as  cold  receptors,  434 

Labyrinth:  see  Vestibular  mechanism 
Labyrinthectomy 

compensation,  562 

eff'ects  of,  561 

species  differences,  562 
Latency 

explanation,  163 

factors  determining,  166 
Lateral   lemniscus:   see   Central    auditory 

function 
Lens 

see  also  Aphakia 

accommodation  and,  660,  664 


age  and,  662 

as  limit  to  accommodation,  664 

nature  of  capsule,  661 

substance 
index,  656 
Light 

spectral  distribution,  707 

unit  of  energy,  665 
Light  intensity 

pigment  migiation  and,  640 
Light,  response  to :  see  Behasior 
Li\er  disorders 

dark  adaptation  and,  690 
Local  response :  see  Subthreshold  response 
Longitudinal  current 

of  axoplasm,  103 

space  and  time  patterns  of,  1 04 
Loudness 

transmission  of,  583 
LSD  25:  see  Lysergic  acid  diethylamide 
Luminance 

definition,  665 
Luster 

conditions  for,  737 
Lux 

definition,  665 
Lyotropic  series 

taste  threshold,  5 1 7 
Lysergic  acid  diethylamide 

alpha  activity  and,  289 

Maculae:  see  Otolith  organs 
Magnesium 

end-plate  potential  and,  208 

p.s.p.  and,  167 
Malononitrile 

steady  potential  and,  318 
Marsilid  :  see  Iproniazide 
Maxwell's  spot 

definition,  666 
Mechanical  energy 

receptor  excitation,  123 
Mechanical  pressure 

pre-  and   postsynaptic   potentials   and, 
302 
Mechanoreceptors,  380-383,  387-426 

see  also  Audition ;  Ear 

discharges  from,  392 

invertebrate,  380 

response  to  thermal  stimulation,  454 

specificity  of,  391 
Medial  lemniscal  system,  396-409 

anatomical  definition,  396 

direct  bulbocortical  pathways,  400 

direct  spinocortical  pathways,  400 

joint  receptors  projection  in,  413 

modality  components,  403 

path    from    dorsal    column    nuclei    to 
\entrobasal  complex,  400 

patterns 

in  dorsal  column  nuclei,  398 


m  projection,  397 
in  response  of  neurons,  405 
in  thalamic  relay  nucleus,  399 
physiological  properties,  397     , 
response,  anesthesia  and,  406 
reticular  activating  system  and,  42 1 
touch-pressure  and,  403 

Medulla  oblongata 
bulbar  relays 

reticular  formation  and,  745 
pain  fibers  in,  487,  488 

Medullary  pyramid 

collateral  activity  of  fibers,  306 

Membrane    action    potential :   see   Action 
potential 

Membrane  current 

space  and  time  patterns  of,  104 
temporal  relation  to   action  potential, 
104 

Membrane  potential 

action  potential  and,  95,  100 
constant  inward  current  and,  1 1 2 
definition,  102 

graded  responsiveness  and,  168 
long  polarizing  currents  and,  i  1 2 
membrane  conductance  and,  103 
membrane  current  and,  93 
membrane  resistance  and,  89 
postsynaptic  potential  and,  161 
rate  of  accommodation  and,  127 
sodium  potential,  [68 
space  and  time  patterns  of,  1 04 
spatial  distribution 

action  potential  and,  103 
threshold,  94 

stimulus  duration  and,  96 
transducer    action    of   synaptic    mem- 
brane and,  156 
true  refractory  period  and,   309 
variation  with  brief  voltage  pulse,   1 00 

Membranes 

electrogenic  evolution,  165 
excitable  and  inexcitable,  154,  155 
impedance  during  activity,  90 
permeability  at  receptors,  143 
resistance  and  after-potential,  1 15 

Menthol 

cold  sensation  due  to,  455 

Mesencephalon 

hearing  and,  589,  590 

pain  fibers  in,  488 

remembered  pain  from  stimulation,  490 

Metrazol :  see  Pentylenetetrazol 

Microelectrodes 

damage  due  to,  270 
double-barrelled,  275 
identification  of  position,  267 
micropipettes  as,  263 
recording  from 
axons,  268 
motoneurons,  271 


772 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


primary  sensory  fibers,  270 

stimulation  through,  276 

types,  262 
Micropipettes 

as  electrodes,  263 

electrical  properties,  265 

preparation,  263 
Modulators,  visual :  see  Vision 
Molluscs 

see  also   Invertebrates 

eye,  camera  style  in,  637 

muscle 

acetylcholine  and,  248 
relaxation  in,  247 

neuromuscular  transmission,  247 
Monaural  stimulation 

definition,  556 
Monoamine  oxidase 

distribution  in  cells,  228 

removal  of  catechol  amines,  228 
Motoneurons 

definition,  272 

model  for  initiation  of  impulses,  274 

single  unit  activity  of,   271-272 

threshold  level,  68 
Muscle 

catch-mechanism    theory    of    contrac- 
tion, 247 

conducted  action  potential,  239 

dual  responses  of,  1 75 

innervation  of,  200-202 

junctional   receptor   function,    209-212 

multiterminal    innervation,    239,    242, 
247 

neuromuscular  transmission,    199-253 

polyneuronal  innervation,  24! 

reciprocal  innervation  of,  181 

smooth 

innervation,  2 1 7 

tetanus  theory,  247 
Muscle,  invertebrate :  see  Invertebrates 
Muscle  potentials 

ciliary  muscle,  662 

in  molluscs,  249 
Muscle  spike:  see  Spike  potentials 
Myelinated  fiber;  see  Nerve  fibers,  mye- 
linated 
Myopia 

correction  for,  655 

definition,  655 

night 

accommodation  and,  664 

sky 

accommodation  and,  664 

Narcotics 

cflFect  on  nodes,  1 1 4 
Nerve  excitability:  see  Nerve  fibers 
Nerve  fibers 

A,  B  and  C,  469 


A,  conduction  in,  393 
after-potentials  of,  i  14-1 17 
blocking  by 

asphyxia,  471,  472 

cocaine,  471 
C,  conduction  in,  394 
conduction  in,  102-1 14 
clectrotonic  state,  1 1  i 
excitability 

determination,  99 

relation  to  threshold,  98 
explanation  of  properties,  64 
group  I-IV,  469 
interaction,  82 
myelinated 

as  cable,  86 

potential  field  of  impulse,  i  i  i 
peripheral,  cutaneous,  393 
potentials  from,  273 
recovery  curve  of,  80 
repetitive  firing,  116 

constant  current  and,  127 

impulse  interval,  127 

membrane  potential  and,  1 1 7 

nerve  accommodation  and,  127 

sensorimotor  cortex,  42 1 

sensory  receptors  and,  127 
rhythmical  activity,  1 1 5 
saltatory  conduction  in,   1 06-1 13 
specificity  of  thermal  response,  444 
temperature  and  activity,  446 
threshold  of,  94-100 
unit  activity  of,  269,  270 
Nerve  impulse,  75-119 

see  also  Conduction;  Transmission 
afferent  discharges 

modification  of,  128 
along  uniform  axon,  102 
character,  79 
conditioning 

end-plate  potential  and,  208 
external  resistance  and,  106 
facilitation  of,  184 
generation  of,  70 
importance  of  local  circuit,  102 
insulating  air  gap  and,  108 
membrane  conductance  of,  89-94 
multiplication,  82 
nodes  of  Ranvier  and,  109 
potential  field,  1 1 1 
propagation,  62 
rate  of  conduction 

fiber  diameter  and,  78 
refractory  period  of,  80 
saltatory   conduction    of,    1 06-1 13 
site  of  initiation,  135 
sodium  theory  of,  62-65,  93,  94,    wi 

119 
spatial  summation  and,  186 
summation,  130 
two-way  conduction,  81 
velocity,  103 


volume  conductor 

potential  field  calculation  and,    105 

Weber-Fechner  law,  1 26 
Nerve  net 

coelenterates,  249 

scyphozoans,  252 
Neuromuscular  junction 

morphology,  200 
Neuromuscular  transmission,  199 

see  also  Cholinergic  transmitter :  Acetyl- 
choline; Transmitter  substances; 
Curare;  Parasympathin 

anticholinesterases  and,  210 

autonomic,  215-235 

chemical  theory,  200 

electrical  theory,  200 

in  coelenterates,  252 

in  crustacean  muscle,  243 

in  insects,  245 

mechanism  of,  211 

skeletal,  1 99-2 1 2 

substance  affecting,  199 

temperature  and,  2 1 1 
Neuron 

see  also  Pyramidal  neurons;  Sensory 
neurons;  Motoneurons  and  parts  of 
the  neuron 

action  in  evoked  potential,  303 

after-discharge  of,  305-308 

autorhythmicity,  308 

differences  in  excitability,  1 7 1 

epileptic  state  in,  342 

excitation  of,  273-276 

excitatory,  71 

impulse  initiation  in,  304 

inhibitory,  71 

internal  structure,  60 

interneuron   single   unit    activity,    272, 

273 

invertebrate,  239-253 

mechanisms  and  behavior,  758 

membrane  of,  61-65 

model  for  initiation  of  impulses,  274 

morphology,  59-61,  257 

postactivity  excitability  of,  308-3 1 1 

single  unit  activity  of,  261-276,  305 

soma 

potentials  from,  273 
Neurons,  axon :  see  Axon ;  Nerve  fibers 
Neuron,  dendrites :  see  Dendrites 
Neuron    theory :    see    Historical    develop- 
ments, concepts 
Neuron  physiology,  59-254 
Neiu'onal  surface  membrane 

electrical  diagram,  62 

function  of,  59 

physiological  properties,  61 

potential  across,  62 

resting  potential,  62 

structure,  60 

transport  across,  60 


INDEX 


773 


Neurotransmitters:   see   Transmitter   sub- 
stances 
Nicotinamide 

visual  excitation  and,  691 
Night  blindness 

opsins  and,  688 

vitamin  A  and,  688 
Night  myopia :  see  Myopia 
Nodal  membrane:  see  Nodes  of  Ranvier, 

membrane 
Nodes  of  Ranvier 

membrane 

threshold  stimulation  and,  96 

role  in  conduction,  109 

threshold  at,  87 
Non-photic  receptors 

in  invertebrates,  369 
Nonpolarizing  competitive  inhibitors:  see 

Synaptic  inactivators 
Noradrenaline:  see  Norepinephrine 
Norepinephrine 

see  also  Adrenergic  transmitter;  Epi- 
nephrine ;  Isopropylnorepinephrine ; 
Dopamine;  Transmitter  substances; 
Catechol  amines 

acetylcholine  and,  210 

as  transmitter  substance,  218 

characterization,  1 78 

content  in  autonomic  nerves,  220 

differentiation  from  epinephrine,  218 

other  adrenergic  transmitters  and,  228 

release,  222 

storage,  221 
Nystagmus 

characteristics  of,  558 

reticular  formation  and,  559 

vestibular  mechanism  and,  558 

Occipital  cortex :  see  Visual  cortex 
Occlusion 

of  evoked  potential 

as  distinct  from  inhibition,  310 
definition,  310 
Ocelli 

classification,  630 

definition,  628 

insect 

retina  in,  631 
Ocular  muscles 

action  of  vestibular  mechanism  on,  559 
Odor  measurement 

methods,  539 
Odorous  substances 

characteristics  of,  538 

chemical  elements  and,  538 
Olfaction,  535-548 

behavior  and,  547 

eflferent  control  of  receptors  for,  745 

enzyme  theories,  539 

olfactory  mucosa  and,  535 

radiation  theories,  539 


receptors  for,  535 

species  specialization  in,   374,  376 

stimulus  intensity  and  response,  536 
Olfactory  bulb 

anesthesia  and,  541,  542 

awakening  reaction  of,  54 1 

central  connections,  543 

central  control,  745 

efferent  pathways,  543 

electrical  activity  in,  540 

evoked  potentials  from,  545 

inhibitory  efferent  fibers,  745 

olfactory  mucosa  and,  537 

organization  of,  537 

patterns  of  activity  in,  540 

removal  of,  544 

spatial  sumnnation  in,  537 

spike  discharge  from,  544 

spike  potentials  from,  545 

spontaneous  activity,  540 
Olfactory  cortex,  543-547 

electrophysiological    investigation,    545 

primary  aiea,  544 

relation  to  rhinencephalon,  546 
Olfactory  mechanisms 

behavior  studies  and,  547 
Olfactory  mucosa 

alkaline  phosphatase  in,  539 

arrangement  of,  535 

connections  with  olfactory  bulb,  537 

smell  sense,  535 
Olfactory  receptors 

anatomy  of,  535 

degeneration    after   olfactory    bulb   re- 
moval, 536 

differentiation  of  response,  542 

innervation,  572 
Olfactory  response,  366 

area  differentiation,  542 

temporal  differentiation,  543 
Ommantidia:  see  Eye,  compound 
Opsin 

see  also  Visual  pigments 

in  visual  excitation,  679 

night  blindness  and,  688 

reaction  with  free  sulfhydryl,  680 
Optic  cortex:  see  Visual  cortex 
Optic  nerve 

central  control,  745 

discharge 

retinal-initiated,  714 

electrical  stimulation,  714 

fiber 

activity  of,  617 

spatial  summation  in,  723 

stimulation 

cortical  areas  responding,  725 
geniculate  response  to,  724 

temporal  summation  in,  723 
Optic  pathway 

anatomy  of,  716 

direct  and  indirect  stimulation,   721 


interaction   of  elements   in,    723 

phenomena  in,  716 

radiation 

cortex  and,  723 

spatial  summation  in,  723 

temporal  summation  in,  723 
Optics,  physiological,  647-691 
Organ  of  Corti 

movements,  572 

structure,  571 
Otoliths 

anatomy  of,  551 

gravity  stimulation,  557 

stimulation  of,  556 


Pacemaker 

artificial  and  natural,  1 1 7 

definition,  1 16 
Pain,  459-502 

see  also  Itching 

abnormal  anatomical  states  and,  475 

abnormal  innersation   for  skin,   467 

adaptation  to,  468 

alpha  rhythm  and,  472 

arterial  constriction  and,  463 

arterial  dilatation  and,  463 

asymbolia,  496 

autonomic  nervous  system  and,  480 

burning,  462,  472 

central  inhibition  of,  499 

chemical  excitants  of,  478 

corneal  stimulus  and,  465 

cortical  ablation  and,  493 

cortical  evoked  potentials  from,  494 

cutaneous 

referred  pain  and,  501 

definition,  459 

diffuse  thalamic  projection  system  and, 

497 
distention  of  \'iscera  and,  463 
double  response,  47 1 

histological  correlates,  473 
due  to  cortical   or  subcortical   lesions, 

492 
due  to  mechanical  stimulation,  467 
due  to  thermal  stimulation,  467 
end  organs,  465 
endocrines  and,  498 
experimental  subjects  for,  464 
fibers  mediating,  468-475 
frequency  of  discharge  and,  468 
frontal  lobectomy  and,  497 
in  second  sensory  area,  495 
indifference,  496 
inflammation  and,  463 
inhibition  and,  499 
ischemia  and,  463 
length  of  discharge  and,  468 
multiple  innervation  and,  476 
parasympathetic  nerves  and,  483 


774 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPH\'S10L00Y    I 


perception 

reticular  formation  and,  736 

pricking,  472 
threshold,  462 

quantitation,  463 

reaction,  496 

reaction  time  to,  473 

receptors,  465-467 

referred,  499-502 

injury  to  central  paths  and,  501 
summation,  500 

reflexes 

lesions  inhibiting,  464 

related  to  itching,  498 

related  to  tickling,  498 

remembered  after  inesencephalic  stim- 
ulation, 490 

representation  in  cerebral  hemispheres, 

493 

second  response  to,  47 1 

significance  to  individual,  460 

somatic  and  visceral  receptors,  467 

sympathetic  nerves  and,  480 

tissue  damage  and,  461 
Pain,    conduction;   see    Pain    fibers,    con- 
duction 
Pain  fibers 

association  with  temperature  fibers,  484 

conduction,  472 
diameter  and,  469 
ischemia  and,  469 

in  anterolateral  column,  486 

in  anterior  roots,  479 

in  cerebral  hemispheres,  492 

in  medulla  oblongata,  487 

in  mesencephalon,  488 

in  posterior  roots,  479 

in  second  sensory  area,  495 

in  spinal  cord,  483 

in  thalamus,  490,  491 

insulation  and,  477 

myelinated  and  unmyelinated,  485 

specificity  for  pain,  461 
Pain  threshold 

constancy,  462 

electrical  stimulation,  462 

nerve  nets  and,  476 

thermal  radiation  and,  461 
Parasympathetic   nervous  system 

cholinergic  transmission  in,  230-233 

pain  and,  483 
Parasympathin 

see  also  Cholinergic  transmitter 

role  in  cholinergic  transmission,  233 
Partial  epilepsy:  see  Epilepsies,  partial 
Pattern  recognition:  see  Vision 
Pelecypods 

see  also    Invertebrates 

ocelli  structure  in,  630 
Penis 

warmth  receptors  in,  444 


Pentylenetetrazol 

pharmacological  efTects  on  synapse,  1 75 
Pentylenetetrazol  seizures 

see  also  Convulsions  generalized.  Strych- 
nine convulsions j   Epilepsy;  Anoxia, 
convulsions 
anoxic  convulsions  and,  340 
cortical  reactivity,  340 
grand   mal  epilepsy  and,   341 
strychnine  convulsions  and,  340 
Pericorpuscular   synaptic   knobs :   see   Sy- 
napse, pericorpuscular  knobs 
Perilymph 

composition,  574 
Peripheral    receptive    fields:    see    Sensory 

systems 
Petit    mal    epilepsy:    we    Epilepsies,    petit 

mal 
Pfliiger's  law  of  contraction:  see  Historical 

development 
Phasic  receptors 
excitation  of,  1 29 
summation,  130 
Phoria :  see  Accommodation 
Photosensitivity 
efficiency  of,  622 
in  ganglia,  624 

in  multicellular  organisms.  624 
in  unicellular  organisms,  623 
peripheral,  624 
Pia-ventricular    potential:    see    DC.    po- 
tentials 
Picrotoxin 

synaptic  transmission  and,  1 78 
Piriform  cortex:  see  Cerebral  cortex 
Pitch:  see  Frequency  discrimination 
Polarization 

see    also    Depolarization;    Postsynaptic 

potentials 
d.c.  potentials  and,  316,  327 
Polarized  light :  see  Arthropods 
Postcentral  cortex :  see  First  somatic  cor- 
tical field 
Postdischarges :  see  Epilepsy 
Posterior    nuclear    thalamic    group :    see 

Thalamus,  posterior  nuclei 
Postexcitatory  depression 
definition,  309 
factor  affecting,  309 
Postsynaptic     membrane:     see     Synapse, 

postsynaptic  membrane 
Postsynaptic  potentials,  65-71,  150-190 
anatomical  considerations,  302 
anoxia  and,  302 
changes  in  amplitudes,  189 
character    and    nature    of   transmitter, 

166 
definition,  149 
depolarizing,  1  73 

central  excitatory  state  and,   164 
hyperpolarization  and,   189 


properties  of,   151 

spatial  considerations,  182 

during  depolarizing,   158 

electrical  stimulation  and,  153 

excitatory,  65,  152 

factors  effecting,  167 

generation  site,  150 

genesis  of,  1 66 

gradation  of,  167 

hyperpolarizing,  151,  158 
central  inhibitory  state,  164 
depolarization  and,  189 
spatial  considerations,  182 

hypothermia  and,  302 

inhibitory,  69,  152 

interrelation  with  spikes,   152,   170 

mechanical  pressure  and,  302 

mode  of  spread,  165 

nature,  150 

pyramidal  neurons  and,  303 

reversal  of,  1 60 

species  differences  in,  171 

standing 

electrotonic  effects,  i8g 
nonpropagated,  164 

transfer  to  electrically  excitable  mem- 
brane, 168 

trauma,  302 

types  of,  151,  172 
Postural  reflexes 

role  of  utricle  in,  557 

vestibular  mechanism  and,  560 
Potassium 

conductance,  62,  118 

excitation  and,  62-65,   '''^'   ''9 
Potassium  potential 

resting  potential  and,  168 
Potassium  theory 

evidence  against,  118 

resting  potential  and,  1 1 7 
Prepotential 

definition,  304 
Presynaptic  impulse 

effects  on  postsynaptic  region,  1 63 
Presynaptic  membrane:  see  Synapse,  pre- 
synaptic membrane 
Presynaptic  potential 

anatomical  considerations,  302 

anoxia  and,  302 

definition,  149 

hypothermia  and,  302 

latency,  162 

mechanical  pressure  and,  302 

trauma  and,  302 
Primary  line  of  sight 

definition,  653 
Primary  receiving  area:  see  First  somatic 

cortical  field 
Proprioceptive:  see  Kinesthetic 
Protopathic  system 

criticism  of,  475 

sensory  mechanism,  391 


INDEX 


775 


Protoplasm 

differential  irritability,  369 
Pulvinar 

sensory  sy terns,  correlation  and,  31 1 
Pupil 

astigmatism  and,  652 

axis 

definition,  653 

cephalopods,  637 

chief  ray  and,  652 

entrance  and  exit,  652 

image  formation  and,  652 
Pupillography 

stray  light  in,  667 
Pure  tone  threshold :  see  Audition 
Purkinje  images 

see  also  Vision 

importance  of,  656 
Purkinje  phenomenon 

see  also  Vision 

description,  682 
Pyramidal  neurons 

collateral  activity  of  fibers,  306 

postsynaptic  impulse  in,  303 

Receptor  cells 

electrical  responses,  165 

functional  components,  165 

generator  potential  and,  127 

in  higher  invertebrates,  37 1 

in  insects,  373 

in  invertebrates,  375 

lines  of  research,  365 

reaction  to  stimuli,  366 
Receptor  potential,  130-135 

absolute  magnitude,  134 

definition,  130,  393 

depression  of,  1 35 

duration  of  stimulus,  134 

from  different  receptors,  131 

impulse  initiation  and,  132 

latency  and,  1 29 

nerve  terminals,  1 30 

phasic  behavior,  131 

procaine  and,  136 

size  of  the  exciting  displacement,  133 

sodium  lack  and,  1 36 

source  of  energy,  143 

stimulus  and,  1 32 

summation  and,  134 

threshold 

depression  and,  135 

tonic  behavior,  131 

velocity  of  the  displacement  and,  133 
Receptors,  123-144 

see  also  various  types  of  receptors 

adaptation  of,  124,  125,  144 

central  control  of,  741-759 

definition,  123 

discharge  frequency  of,  126-128 

electronmicroscopy  of,   141 

excitation  of,   123,   129,   130 


excitation  reduction  and,    126 

initiation  of  impulse,  142 

invertebrate 

nonphotic,  369-383 
photic,  621-642 

junctional,  209 

minute  structure  of,   141,   142 

potentials  of,  130-135 

repetitive  firing,  127 

role,  123 

sensitivity  and  time  factors,   124 

sodium  lack  and,  136,  137 

specific  sensitivity,  1 24 

time  course,  129 

tonic,  126 

transmitter  action  on,  139-141 
Recruitment 

repetitive  stimulation  and,  31  i 
Reflexes 

see  also  individual  reflexes 

synaptic  determinants  of,  147-194 
Refraction,  654-656 

coincidence  optometer,  659 

indices  of 

measurements,  656 

retinoscope  and,  659 
Refractory  period 

barbiturates  and,  308 

ionic  hypothesis  and,  64 

visual  cortex,  308 
Repetitise  firing :  see  Nerve  fibers 

current  theories,  117 

definition,  83 

equilibrium  potential  and,   162 

external  K  concentration  and,  1 1 7 

potassium  potential  and,  168 
Reticular  activating  system;  see  Reticular 

formation 
Reticular  formation 

activation,  42 1 

anoxic  convulsions,  339 

ascending  sensory  paths  and,  752 

as  functional  unit,  42 1 

as    origin    of   generalized    convulsions, 

335 
ascending  sensory  paths  and,  752 
attention  and,  367 
auditory  pathway  and,  591 
behavior  and,  755 
bulbar  relays  and,  745 
cerebral  afferent  systems  and,  747-749 
control  of  afferent  paths  by,  745-747 
dorsal  column  relays  and,  745 
efferent    effects    upon    retinal    activity, 

745 
efferent  pathways  and,  757 
hearing  and,  591 
inhibition  of  spinal  relays,  746 
medial  lemniscal  system  and,  42 1 
pain  perception  and,  756 
rapid  phase  of  nystagmus  and,  559 
role  in  tonic  and  clonic  spasms,  340 


sensory  system,  correlation  and,  3 1 1 

spinal  ascending  relays  and,  745 

spinal  motor  activity  and,  561 

spinothalamic  tract  and,  421 

stretch  receptors  and,  743 

vestibular  stimulation  and,  561 
Retina,  665-669,  671-710 

see    also     Cones;     Rods;     Electroretin- 
ography;  Ocelli 

action  potentials  from,  617 

conduction  across  surface,  696 

conjugate  focus,  658 

damage  and  ERG,  700 

efferent  control  of,  745 

electrical  activity  of,  696-704,  710 

entoptic  phenomena,  669 

histology  of,  693-696 

illuminance  of,  665,  669 

inhibition  in,  706 

neural  activity  in,  693-710 

on-system  and  off-system  in,  705 

receptive  fields  and  adaptation,  705 

spike  discharge,  704 

stimulation 

cortical  localization  and,  726 

type  and  ERG,  698 

vitamin  A  and,  68g 
Retinal  image,  647-691 

see  also  Image  formation 

blur,  667 

size,  definition,  654 
Retinene 

see  also  Visual  pigments 

rhodopsin  and,  674 
Retinitis  pigmentosa 

vitamin  A  and,  691 
Rhinencephalon 

EEG  and  electrical  stimulation  of,  351 

epilepsy,  partial  and,  350 

olfactory  cortex  and,  546 
Rhodopsin,  672-676 

see  also  Visual  pigments 

adaptation  and,  686 

anagenesis,  673 

as  visual  pigment,  672 

bleaching  and  resynthesis,  686 

changes  during  vision,  672 

neogenesis,  673 

retinene  and,  674 

scoptic  sensitivity  and,  682 

synthetic  system,  674 

vitamin  A  and,  672,  674 
Righting  reflex 

mechanism  of,  560 
Rods 

see  also  Retina 

electroretinogi'am  and,  699 

flicker  fusion  and,  708 

histology  of,  693 

modulators  in,  708 

sensitivity  of,  699 

visual  pigments  in,  671 


776 


HANDBOOK    OF    PHYSIOLOGY 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


Roller's  nucleus 

equilibrium  and,  558 
Rotation 

perception  of,  554 
Ruffini  end  organs 

as  warm  receptors,  434 

Saccule 

anatomy  of,  551 

function,  557 
Saltatory    conduction:    see    Conduction, 

saltatory 
Salty  taste 

substances  giving,  3 1 6 
Scheiner  principle 

accommodation  and,  659 
Schwalbe's  nucleus 

equilibrium  and,  558 
Sceptic  adaptation 

electroretinogram  and,  699 

retinal  sensiti%ity  in,  699 
Second  sensory  area 

conscious  pain  sensation,  495 

in  man,  494 
Second  somatic  area 

relation  to  spinothalamic  system,   418 
Seizures,   general :   see   Convulsions,   gen- 
eralized; Epilepsy 
Self-selection  studies 

see  also  Behavior,  Appetitive  behavior. 
Conditioned  reflex 

intragastric  osmotic  pressure  and,  529 

taste  and,  527 
Semicircular  canals,  553-556 

action  of,  553 

adequate  stimulation  to,  553 

anatomy  of,  550 

bidirectional  function,  554 

endolymph  flow  in,  553 

functions,  549 

hydrodynamic  theory,  553 

inadequate  stimulus,  556 

stimulation  of,  553-556 
Sensorimotor  cortex 

repetitive  stimulation  and,  42 1 
Sensory  cortex:  see  Sensory  systems 
Sensory  nerve  fibers 

direct  stimulation  of,  45*2 

peripheral,  468 
Sensory  neurons 

inhibition  of,  379 

scheme  for  proprioception,  379 
Sensory  plexuses 

cutaneous,  467 

subcutaneous,  467 
Sensory  reaction  time:  see  Sensory  systems 
Sensory  systems,  365-759 

see  also  Receptors;  specific  systems 

central  control  of,  741-759 

correlation,  3 1 1 

cortex 

periodic  excitability  change,  309 


electrophysiological  methods,  389 

interaction  in,  752 

pathways 

anesthesia  and,  752,  755 

as  determined  by  lesions,  420 

central  control  of,  741 

peripheral  receptive  fields 

projection  upon  central  neurons,  405 
size,  404 

receptors,  see  Receptors 

reticular  formation  and,  421 

stimulus  intensity 

reaction  time  and,  473 
Sensory  units 

description,  123 

patterns  of  information,  1 25 

receptise  fields,  125 

steady  states  and,  1 26 
Serotonin :  see  5-Hydroxytryptamine 
Skin 

abnormal  pain  innervation,  467 

analgesic  spot,  466 

as  thermopile  bolometer,  442 

conduction  of  heat,  437 

sensations  from,  390-394 

sensory  plexuses  in,  467 

temperature  gradient  in,  453 

thermosensitive  areas,  43 1 
Skin  receptors 

reaction  to  stimuli,  366 
Skin  temperature 

change  and  adaptation,  439 
Sky  myopia :  see  Myopia 
Smell:  see  Olfaction 
Smooth  muscle :  see  Muscle 
Sodium  conductance 

membrane  potential 
changes  and,  62 
Sodium  ions 

acetylcholine  action  and,  210 

receptor  potentials  and,  136,  137 

relation  to  excitation,  62-65,93,94,  1 18, 

"9 

relation  to  inhibition,  70 
Sodium  potential 

membrane  potential  and,  168 
Sodium  theory 

action  potential  and,  118,  119 

proof  of,  62-65 
Solid  angle 

definition,  665 
Somatic    afferent    systems:    see    Sensory 

systems 
•Somesthetic  discrimination 

postcentral  cortex  and,  425 
Sound  stimulation 

of  labyrinth,  557 
Sour  taste 

electric  current  and,  523 

pH  and,  513 
Spatial  summation 

in  olfactory  bulb,  537 


m  optic  nerve,  723 

in  optic  pathway,  723 

synaptic,  66 
Spike  potentials 

antidromic,  67 

invertebrate  muscle 

quarternary  ammonium  compounds 
and,  244 

IS  spike,  67 

SD  spike,  67 
Spinal  cord 

see  also  Dorsal  columns 

anterolateral  column   and   pain   fibers, 
486 

ascending  relays 

central  control  of,  745-747 
reticular  formation  and,  745 

association  pain  and  temperature  fibers, 
484 

lesions,  pain  and,  464 

neurons,  periodic  excitability,  310 

pain  fibers  in,  479,  483-487 
Spinothalamic  system,  415-419,  484-492 

as  sensory  path,  415 

ipsilateral  pathways,  419 

modaUty  organization,  418 

origin,  4 1 7 

pain  paths  in,  484-492 

posterior  nuclear  thalamic  group  and, 
418 

reticular  activating  system  and,  42 1 

second  somatic  area  and,  148 

tactile  fibers  in,  416 

termination,  417 

topical  organization,  418 
Spreading  depression 

conditions  for  production,  324 

cortical  maturity  and,  324 

d.c.  changes  and,  323-325 

species  variation,  324 

spikes  and,  67 
Static  stimuli:  see  Equilibrium 
Statocysts 

invertebrate,  382,  383 
Steady  potential:  see  DC.  potentials 
Stiles-Crawford  effect 

definition,  666 
Stray  light 

eye  and,  648 

pupillography  in,  667 

source  for  eye,  667 
Strength-duration  relation 

anatomical  determinants,  98 

Blair's  equation  for,  97 

formula,  98 
Stretch  receptors 

central  control  of,  743,  744 

efferent  control  of,  743 
Striate  cortex 

see  Visual  cortex 


INDEX 


777 


Strychnine 

EEG  and,  348 

inhibition  of  transmission,  71 
partial  epilepsy  and,  348 
postexcitatory  depression  and,  309 
synaptic  transmission  and,  1 75 
tetanus,  561 
Strychnine  convulsions 

see      also     Pentylenetetrazol     seizures; 
Convulsions,    generalized;    Epilepsy; 

Anoxia,  convulsions 
cortex,  reactivity,  340 
electrical  discharges  during,  339 
reticular  discharge  during,  339 
theory  of,  344 
Subthreshold  response 
action  potential  and,  98 
area  hypothesis,  98 
membrane  potential  and,  98 
spatial  factor 

time  course  and,  98 
theory,  76 
Sugars 

order  of  sweetness,  520 
Summation:      see     Spatial      summation. 

Temporal  summation;   Pain;   Ner\e 

impulse 
Sweet  taste 

effect  of  drugs,  520 
molecular  structure  and,  518 
order  in  sugars,  520 
stereoisomerism  and,  519 
substances  giving,  518 
Sympathetic  nervous  system 

adrenergic  transmission  in,  218-229 
pain  and,  480-483 
touch  receptors  and,  742 
Sympathins 

see  Adrenergic  transmitter 
Synapses,  147-194 

comparative  physiology  of,  1 71-175 

definition,  150 

electrically   excitable   and  unexcitable, 

192 
electrogenesis  by,  153-165 
electrotonic  effects  across,  163 
excitability,  65 
function,  60 
inhibitory,  65 

strychnine  and,  71 

tetanus  toxin  and,  71 
integrative  activity,  182 
membrane 

augmented  responsiveness,  169 

chemical  sensitivity,  163 

postsynaptic,  definition,  60 
transducer  action,  154,  157,  161 
pericorpuscular  knobs 

postsynaptic  discharge  and,  303 
pharmacological  properties,  175-182 
postsynaptic  membrane 

sustained  electrogenesis  and,   156 


postsynaptic  potentials,  150-175 

presynaptic  membrane 
function,  60 

spatial  interrelations,  182 

structure  of,  61 

transmitter  specificity,  181 
Synaptic  activators 

characterization,  1 75 

mode  of  action,  163,  180 
Synaptic  block 

barbiturates  and,  301 

repetitive  stimulation  and,  301 
Synaptic  delay 

definition,  170 

explanation,  170 
Synaptic  electrogenesis:  see  Electrogenesis 
Synaptic  inactivators 

characterization,  163 

mode  of  action,  1 80 
Synaptic  inhibitors 

characterization,  175 
Synaptic  membrane:  see  .Synapse,  mem- 
brane 
Synaptic  transmission,  147-194 

see  also   Ephaptic  transmission.  Trans- 
mission 

chemical,  150 

compared  to  conduction,  149 

compared  to  ephaptic,  149 

effectiveness  of,  183 

electric  current  and,  67 

events  in,  165 

integrative  function  of,  182-190 

Tactile  activity 

postcentral  fields  and,  423 
Tactile  fibers 

in  spinothalamic  system,  416 
Tactile  stimuli 
definition,  388 

fibers  of  different  size  and,  393 
specificity  of  receptors,  391 
Tactile  system 

see    also    Touch -pressure    system;    Cu- 
taneous stimuli;  Skin  receptors 
central  representation,  395 
Talbot 

definition  of,  665 
Talbot  effect 

description  of,  730 
Taste,  507-547 
adaptation,  524 
cross,  525 

enhanced  sensitivity  and,  525 
area  stimulated,  524 
behavior  and,  527 
buds 

anatomical  sites,  507 
chemical  stimuli  and,  510 
CNS  pathways,  509 
deficit 

ablation  or  section  and,  510 


definition,  507 
duration  of  stimulus,  524 
effect  of  mixtures,  526 
fibers 

pathway  to  CNS,  509 
sensitivity  pattern,  51 1 
ingestion  and,  529 
intensity  relations,  525 
reaction  time,  524 
receptor  anatomy,  507-509 
receptor  mechanisms,  510 
reinforcement  of  conditioning  by,  529 
self-selection  studies  and,  527 
sensiti\'ity 

drugs  and,  510 

influence  of  blood  constituents,  529 
mechanisms  of  stimulation,  513 
species  differences,  51 1 
specialization  in  animals,  376 
specificity 

drugs  and,  520 
sites  on  cell  membrane,  512 
temperature  and,  523 
Taste  blindness 

chemical  structure  and,  52 1 
inheritance,  52 1 
Taste  threshold 
cation  series,  51 7 
measures  of,  513 
sodium  salts  anion  series,  517 
Temperature 

brain  excitability  and,  308 
change 

thermal  receptors  and,  449 
gradient 

spatial  and  temporal  aspects,  442 
nerve  fiber  activity  and,  446 
taste  sense,  523 
Temperature     sensibility:     see     Thermal 

sensations 
Temporal  information :  see  Audition 
Temporal  summation 
in  optic  nerve,  723 
in  optic  pathway,  723 
Tetanus  toxin 

inhibition  of  transmission,  71 
Tetraethylammonium  chloride 

action  potential  and,  101 
Thalamocaudate    inhibitory    system:    see 

Thalamus 
Thalamic    relay    nucleus:    see   Thalamus, 

relay  nuclei 
Thalamus 

caudate  inhibitory  system  and,  344 
diffuse  projection  system 

pain  and,  497 
nuclei 

auditory  cortex  and,  598 
pain  fibers  in,  490 
petit  mal  and,  337 
posterior  nuclei 

as  part  of  spinothalamic  system,    418 


778 


HANDBOOK    OF    PH'lSIOLOOY  ' 


NEUROPHYSIOLOGY    I 


relay  nuclei 

pathway  to  dorsal  column  n\iclci,  400 
patterns  in,  399 

reticular  formation  and,  747 

tactile  area 
patterns,  401 

thermoreceptive  neurons  in,  435 
Thermal  energy 

receptor  excitation  by,  1 24 
Thermal  fibers,  444-455 

see  also  Cold  fibers 

association  with  pain  fibers,  484 

discharge,  446-453 

discharge  and  temperature,  447,  448 

latency  to  cooling,  452 

paradoxical  discharge,  452 

temperature  change  and,  449,  450 
Thermal  receptors,  431-457 

acetylcholine  and,  455 

adaptation,  456 

afferent  ner\'e  paths  of,  435 

carbon  dioxide,  455 

cold-blooded  animals,  445 

depth  in  skin,  432 

excitation  mechanism,  456,  457 

identification,  434 

intracutaneous    temperature    gradient, 

453 

invertebrates,  379,  380 

ischemia,  442,  443,  453 

location  of,  431-435 

paradoxical  responses,  443 

specificity  of  fibers,  444 

temperature  change  and,  449 
Thermal  sensations,  431-457 

central  threshold  for,  455 

cold 

non-thermal  causes  of,  454 
paradoxical,  452 

Ebbecke  phenomenon,  443 

hot,  444 

temperature  change  in  skin,  438 

topography,  43 1 ,  432 

warmth 

paradoxical,  453 

Weber's  deception,  454 

Weber's  phenomena,  453 

Weber's  theory,  437,  443 
Thermal  thresholds 

temperature  change  and,  440 
Threshold  membrane  potential ;  see  Mem- 
brane potential 
Threshold     receptor     potential:     see     Re- 
ceptor potential 
Tickling 

as  related  to  pain,  498,  499 
Tonal  pattern  discrimination 

auditory  cortical  ablation  and,  597 
Tone  frequency 

response  to,  by  cochlear  ncrscs,  604 
Tonic  labyrinthine  reflexes 

action  of,  560 


otoliths  and,  560 
Tonic  neck  reflexes 

action  of,  560 

otoliths  and,  560 
Touch-pressure  system,  387-426 

see  also  Cutaneous  sensations;  Tactile 
system;  Skin  receptors 

adaptation  in,  403 

in  deep  fascia,  415 

medial  lemniscal  system  and,  403 
Touch  receptors 

invertebrate,  380 

sympathetic  influence,  742 
Transducer  action 

synaptic  membranes  and,  154,  156,  157, 
161 

synaptic  electrogenesis  and,   189 

tactile  receptors  and,  380 
Transmission 

see  also  Ephaptic  transmission;  Synaptic 
transmission;  Nerve  impulse;  Con- 
duction 

nerve  conduction  and,  62,  65 

between  neurons,  65 

electrical  versus  chemical,  2 1  7 

integrative  activity  and,  149 

neuromuscular,  199-253 
autonomic,  215-235 
invertebrate,  239-253 
skeletal,  199-235 

postsynaptic  potential  and,  149 
Transmission,    autonomic    neuroeffector : 
see  Transmitter  substances;  Chemical 
transmission 
Transmittance,  spectral 

eye  and,  666 
Tiansmitter  substances 

ee  also  Adrenergic  transmitter;  Cho- 
linergic transmitter 

action  Ca  on,  153 

action  Mg  on,  153 

blood  content  of,  234 

characterization,  178 

crustacean,  243 

desensitization  at  synapse,  157 

excitatory,  7 1 

histamine,  141 

identification,  178 

inhibitory,  71 

insect,  247 

localized  action,  181 

mode  of  action,  166,  180,  233 

molluscs,  248 

requirements,  1 79 

synaptic  specificity,  181 

urine  content  of,  234 
Triad  response 

definition,  662 
Tympanic  membrane 

function,  568 
Tympanal  organs 

in  invertebrates,  382 


Tympanic  reflex 

characterization,  568 

Ultraviolet  light 

aphakic  eye  and,  666 

eye  sensitivity  to,  641 
Unicellular  organisms 

photosensitivity  in,  623 
Utricle 

anatomy  of,  550 

function,  557 

Vestibular  mechanism,  549-562 

see  also  Equilibrium;  Labyrinthectomy 

action  on  ocular  muscles,  559 

adaptation,  555 

anatomy  of  labyrinth,  550 

ascending  pathways  from,  558-560 

caloric  stimulation.  556 

connections  with  brain,  558 

cortical  projection,  559 

destruction  of,  561,  562 

discharge  after  stimulation,  555 

galvanic  stimulation,  556 

mode  of  action  of,  552 

muscle  contraction  and,  561 

nystagmus  and,  558 

postural  reflexes  and,  560 

receptor  cells,  552 

reticular  formation  and,  561 

reflexes  from,  558-561 

sound  stimulation  of,  557 
Vestibular  nerve 

electrical  stimulation  of,  559 

origin,  552 
Vestibular  nuclei 

connections,  558 

fiber  paths  from,  558 
Vestibulospinal  tract 

anatomy  of,  560 
Vibration  sense 

human,  374 

insect,  374 

invertebrate,  380 
Vibrational  stimulation 

of  utricle,  557 
Vision,  617-759 

see  also  Brightness  vision;  Image  forma- 
tion; Retinal  image;  Eye;  Retina; 
Cones;  Rods 

alternation  of  response  theory,  734 

apparent  movement,  737 

binocular  rivalry,  737 

brightness  contrast  and,  737 

central  mechanisms,  713-739 
modes  of  study,  7 1 4 

color  vision,  738,  739 

cortical  facilitation,  733 

cortical  on-off  responses,  730 

definition  of,  713,  714 

flicker,  729-732 


INDEX 


779 


foveal 

macula  lutea  and,  666 

geniculate  facilitation,  733 

implicit  time  and  stimulus  area,  728 

modes  of  study,  7 1 4 

modulators 

as  absorption  curses,  708 
in  cones  and  rods,  708 

movement,  738 
description  of,  715 

pattern  recognition,  641 

primary  line  of  sight,  653 

problems  of,  715 

real  movement  and,  737 

retinal  image  formation  and,  647-691 

solid  angle  and,  665 

striate  cortex  and,  727 

sustained  potentials 

apparent  movement  and,  738 
dendritic  activity  and,  735 

sustained  potentials  in,  729,  735 

triad  response,  662 
Visual  accommodation :  see  Accommoda- 
tion 
Visual  cortex,  719-725 

ablation  in  monkey,  727 

activation  of  neurons  in,  727 

dendrite  behavior  in,  726 

latency  to  spectral  stimuli,  739 

periodic  excitability  change,  309 

postexcitatory  depression  and,  309 

refractory  periods,  308 

response  to  stimuli,  719,  738 

retinal  stimulation  and,  726 

subcortical  pathways  to,  726 


Visual  excitation 

absorption  spectra  and,  682 

chemistry  and,  671,  679 

enzymes  and,  673 

nicotinamide  and,  691 

opsin  in,  679 

spectral  sensitivity  and,  682 

vitamin  A  and,  691 
Visual  fields 

definition  of,  664 

limits,  665 
Visual  pigments,  671-691 

ief  also   Cyanopsin;    lodopsin;   Opsin; 
Retinene;  Rhodopsin 

adaptation  and,  684 

alcohol  dehydrogenase,  673 

chemistry  of,  67 1 

cones  and,  67 1 

cyanopsin  as,  678 

DPN  and,  673 

in  eye,  617 

porphyropsin  as,  676 

rhodopsin  and,  672,  678 

rods  and,  67  i 

\isual  sensitivity  and,  685 
Visual  purple :  see  Rhodopsin 
Visual  system,  central,  713-739 

bilateral  function  in,  736 

cortical  response  to  stimuli,  719 

habituation 

experimental  production,  754 

interaction  with  auditory  impulses,  31 1 

receptors 

organization  of,  705 

recruitment  in,  311 


sensitivity 

visual  pigment  and,  685 

spatial  summation,  723 

spectral  stimulation  and,  738 

stimulus  area 

and  implicit  time,  728 

temporal  summation,  723,  724 
Vitamin  A 

night  blindness  and,  688 

retinal  integrity  and,  689 

retinitis  pigmentosa  and,  691 

rhodopsin  and,  672,  674 

visual  excitation  and,  691 

visual  pigments  and,  676 
Vitreous  humor 

index,  656 
Volume  conductor ;  see  Ner\"c  impulse 

Warm  fibers 

see  also  Thermal  fibers 

discharge,  448 

discharge  and  temperature,  448 

latency  to  cooling,  452 

temperature  change  and,  450 
Warm  threshold:  see  Thermal  thresholds 
Warmth  sensation :  see  Thermal  sensations 
Weber-Fechner  law:  see  Nerve  impulse 
Weber's  deception :  see  Thermal  sensations 
Weber's  phenomenon:  see  Thermal  sen- 
sations 
Weber's  theory :  see  Thermal  sensations 
Wedensky  inhibition 

definition,  159 

explanation,  159 


"o^VcaT