Skip to main content

Full text of "The integrative action of the nervous system"

See other formats


3 


n 


YALE  UNIVERSITY  : 

I 

MRS.   HEPSA  ELY  SILLIMAN  MEMORIAL  LECTURES  1 


THE  INTEGRATIVE  ACTION  OF  THE 
NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


SILLIMAN    MEMORIAL   LECTURES 
PUBLISHED    BY    YALE    UNIVERSITY   PRESS 


ELECTRICITY  AND  MATTER.  By  Joseph  John  Thomsok,  d.sc.ll.d., 
PH.D.,  F.R.8.,  Fellow  of  Trinity  College  and  Cavendish  Professor  of 
Experimental  Physics,  Cambridge  University.    {FouHh  Printing.) 

THE  INTEGRATIVE  ACTION  OF  THE  NERVOUS  SYSTEM.  By 
CHABLK8  S.  Sherrington,  d.sc,  m.d..  hon.  ll.d.  tor.,  f.r.s.,  Holt  Pro- 
fessor of  Physiology,  University  of  Liverpool.    {Sixth  Printing.) 

RADIOACTIVE  TRANSFORMATIONS.  By  Ernest  Rutherford,  d.sc, 
LL.D.,  F.R.S. ,  Macdonald  Professor  of  Physics,  McGill  University. 
(Second  Printing.) 

EXPERIMENTAL  AND  THEORETICAL  APPLICATIONS  OF  THER- 
MODYNAMICS TO  CHEMISTRY.  By  Dr.  Walther  Nernst,  Pro- 
fessor and  Director  of  the  Institute  of  Physical  Chemistry  in  the 
University  of  Berlin. 

PROBLEMS  OF  GENETICS.  By  William  Batesov,  m.a.,  f.r.s..  Director 
of  the  John  Innes  Horticultural  Tnstitution,  Merton  Park,  Surrey, 
England.    {Second  Printing.) 

STELLAR  MOTIONS.    With  Special  Reference  to  Motions  Determined 
by  Means  of  the  Spectrograph.  By  William  Wallace  Campbell,  sc.d., 
LL.D.,  Director  of  the  Lick  Observatory,  University  of  California. 
{Second  Printing.) 

THEORIES  OF  SOLUTIONS.  By  Svante  Arrhenius,  ph.d.,  sc.d.,  m.d.. 
Director  of  the  Physico-Chemical  Department  of  the  Nobel  Institute, 
Stockholm,  Sweden.    {Third  Printing.) 

IRRITABILITY.  A  Physiological  Analysis  of  the  General  Effect  of 
Stimuli  in  Living  Substances.  By  Max  Verworn.  m.d.,  ph.d.,  IYo- 
fessor  at  Bonn  Physiological  Institute.    (Second  Printing.) 

PROBLEMS  OF  AMERICAN  GEOLOGY.     By  William  North  Rice, 
Frank  D.  Adams,  Arthur  P.  Coleman,  Charles  D.  Walcott,  Waldemar 
LiNDGREN,  Frederick  Leslie  Ransome,  and  William  D.  Matthew. 
{Second  Printing.) 

THE  PROBLEM  OF  VOLCANISM.  By  Joseph  Paxson  Iddings,  ph.b., 
BCD.    {Second  Printing.) 

ORGANISM  AND  ENVIRONMENT  AS  ILLUSTRATED  BY  THE 
PHYSIOLOGY  OF  BREATHING.  By  John  Scott  Haldane,  m.d.. 
LL.D.,  F.R.S.,  Fellow  of  New  College,  Oxford  University.  {Second 
Printing.) 

A  CENTURY  OF  SCIENCE  IN  AMERICA:  WITH  SPECIAL  REFER- 
ENCE TO  THE  AMERICAN  JOURNAL  OF  SCIENCE.  1818-1918.  By 
Edward  Salisbury  Dana,  ph.d.,  Charles  Schuchert,  ll.d..  Herbert  E. 
Gregory,  ph.d.,  the  Late  Joseph  Barbell,  ph.d.,  sc.d.,  George  Otis 
Smith,  vh.d..  Director  of  the  United  States  Geological  Stirvey, Richard 
SwANN  Lull,  ph.d.,  Louis  V.  Pirsson,  m.a.,  William  E.  Ford,  ph.d., 
R.  B.  SosMAN,  PH.D.,  Physicist  at  the  Geophysical  Laboratory  of  the 
Carnegie  Institution,  Horace  L.  Wells,  sc.d.,  Harry  W.  Foote,  ph. 
D.,  Leigh  Page,  ph.d.,  Wesley  R.  Coe,  ph.d.,  and  George  L.  Goodale, 

A.M.,  M.D.,  LL.D. 


M 


The  Integrative  Action  of 
the  Nervous  System 


By 

Charles  S.  Sherrington 

D.SC.,  M.D.,  HON.  LL.D.TOR.  ,  F.  R.  S. 

HoU  Prqfessor  qf  Physiology  in  the  University  qf  Liverpool, 
Honorary  Member  qflke  American  Physiological  Society,  etc. 


WITH  ILLUSTRATIONS 


I.  M.  ai-i 


NEW  HAVEN:  YALE  UNIVERSITY  PRESS 

LONDON:  HUMPHREY  MILFORD 

OXFORD  UNIVERSITY  PRESS 

MDCCCCXX 


Copyright,  1906 
By  Yale  University 


First  published,  October,  1906 

Second  printing,  October,  1911 

Third  printing,  April,  1914 

Fourth  printing,  November,  1916 

Fifth  printing,  January,  1918 

Sixth  printing,  December,  1920 


QP 

2)55 
S5S- 


To 
DAVID    FERRIER 

IN   TOKEN  OF   RECOGNITION   OF  HIS   MANY   SERVICES 

TO 

THE  EXPERIMENTAL  PHYSIOLOGY 

OF 

THE  CENTRAL  NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


THE    SILLIMAN    FOUNDATION 

In  the  year  1883  a  legacy  of  eighty  thousand  dollars 
was  left  to  the  President  and  Fellows  of  Yale  College  in 
the  city  of  New  Haven,  to  be  held  in  trust,  as  a  gift 
from  her  children,  in  memory  of  their  beloved  and  hon- 
ored mother  Mrs.  Hepsa  Ely  Silliman. 

On  this  foundation  Yale  College  was  requested  and 
directed  to  establish  an  annual  course  of  lectures  de- 
signed to  illustrate  the  presence  and  providence,  the 
wisdom  and  goodness  of  God,  as  manifested  in  the  natural 
and  moral  world.  These  were  to  be  designated  as  the 
Mrs.  Hepsa  Ely  Silliman  Memorial  Lectures.  It  was 
the  belief  of  the  testator  that  any  orderly  presentation 
of  the  facts  of  nature  or  history  contributed  to  the  end 
of  this  foundation  more  effectively  than  any  attempt  to 
emphasize  the  elements  of  doctrine  or  of  creed ;  and  he 
therefore  provided  that  lectures  on  dogmatic  or  polemical 
theology  should  be  excluded  from  the  scope  of  this  foun- 
dation, and  that  the  subjects  should  be  selected  rather 
from  the  domains  of  natural  science  and  history,  giving 
special  prominence  to  astronomy,  chemistry,  geology,  and 
anatomy. 

It  was  further  directed  that  each  annual  course  should 
be  made  the  basis  of  a  volume  to  form  part  of  a  series 
constituting  a  memorial  to  Mrs.  Silliman.  The  memo- 
rial fund  came  into  the  possession  of  the  Corporation 
of  Yale  University  in  the  year  1902;  and  the  present 
volume  constitutes  the  second  of  the  series  of  memorial 
lectures.  The  first  volume  in  this  series  was  "Electricity 
and  Matter,"  by  Prof.  J.  J.  Thomson,  of  Cambridge 
University. 


PREFACE 

The  pressure  of  varied  work  has  prevented  my  forward- 
ing the  text  of  these  lectures  for  publication  so  early  as 
I  could  have  wished,  and  I  take  this  occasion  of  express- 
ing my  regret  at  the  delay.  The  circumstance  that 
thus  impels  me  to  preface  with  a  few  words  their  issue 
affords  me  also  the  opportunity  of  recording  how  much 
I  am  indebted  to  President  Hadley  and  the  authorities 
of  Yale  University  for  their  kindness  during  a  visit 
which  I  shall  ever  remember  with  pleasure.  To  Pro- 
fessor Chittenden,  Director  of  the  Sheffield  Scientific 
School  of  the  University,  I  owe  further  a  debt  of  grati- 
tude for  unstinted  assistance  open  to  me  from  him  on 
all  occasions. 

C.  S.  s. 


Page 


CONTENTS 


LECTURE    I 

INTRODUCTORY— CO-ORDINATION  IN  THE  SIMPLE 

REFLEX ' I 

Argument :  The  nervous  system  and  the  integration  of  bodily 
reactions.  Characteristics  of  integration  by  nervous  agency. 
The  unit  mechanism  in  integration  by  the  nervous  system  is 
the  reflex.  Co-ordination  of  reflexes  one  with  another.  Co- 
ordination in  the  simple  reflex.  Conduction  in  the  reflex-arc. 
Function  of  the  receptor  to  lower  for  its  reflex-arc  the  threshold 
value  of  one  kind  of  stimulus  and  to  heighten  the  threshold 
value  of  all  other  kinds  of  stimuli  for  that  arc :  it  thus  confers 
selective  excitability  on  the  arc.  Differences  between  con- 
duction in  nerve-trunks  and  in  reflex-arcs  respectively.  These 
probably  largely  referable  to  the  intercalation  of  synaptic  mem- 
branes in  the  conductive  mechanism  of  the  arc.  Latent  time 
of  reflexes.  Reflex  latency  inversely  proportional  to  intensity 
of  stimulation.  Latency  of  initial  and  incremental  reflexes. 
None  of  the  latent  interval  consumed  in  establishing  connec- 
tion between  the  elements  of  a  resting  arc.  After- discharge  a 
characteristic  of  reflex  reactions.  Increase  of  after-discharge 
by  intensification  of  the  stimulus,  or  by  prolongation  of  short 
stimuH.     "  Inertia  "  and  "  momentum  "  of  reflex-arc  reactions. 

LECTURE    II 

CO-ORDINATION  IN  THE  SIMPLE  REFLEX  {continued)  36 
Argument :  Reflex-arcs  show  high  capacity  for  summing  excita- 
tions. Irreversibility  of  direction  of  conduction  in  reflex-arcs. 
Reversibility  of  direction  of  conduction  in  certain  nerve-nets, 
e.  g.  that  of  Medusa.  Independence  between  the  rhythm  of 
the  reflex-discharge  and  the  rhythm  of  the  external  stimulus 
exciting  it.  Refractory  phase  in  reflexes  ;  in  the  eyelid-reflex ; 
in  the  scratch-reflex.  The  neuronic  construction  of  the  reflex- 
arc  of  the  scratch-reflex.  Long  descending  proprio-spinal 
tracts  revealed  by  the  method  of  "successive  degeneration." 
The  "final  common  path"  and  the  "afferent  arc."  Intra- 
spinal seat  of  the  refractory  phase  of  the  scratch-reflex. 
The  value  of  refractory  phase  in  the  co-ordination  of  the 
swimming  of  Medusa.  Its  value  in  the  co-ordination  of 
the  scratch-reflex.  Significance  of  the  intraspinal  situation  of 
the  refractory  phase  of  the  scratch-reflex.  Other  instances 
of  "central"  refractory  phase. 


xii  CONTENTS 

LECTURE    III 

Page 
CO-ORDINATION  IN  THE  SIMPLE  REFLEX  {concluded)       70 

Argument:  Correspondence  between  intensity  of  stimulus  and 
intensity  of  reflex  reaction.  Differences  between  different  re- 
flexes in  this  respect.  Functional  solidarity  of  the  intraspinal 
group  of  elements  composing  a  reflex  "centre."  Sensitivity 
of  reflexes,  as  compared  with  nerve-trunks,  to  asphyxial  and 
anaemic  conditions,  and  to  anaesthetic  and  certain  other  drugs. 
Functional  significance  of  the  neural  perikarya.  Reflexes 
of  double-sign.  Reflexes  of  successive  double-sign,  and  of 
simultaneous  double-sign.  Evidence  of  reciprocal  innervation 
in  reflexes.  Reflex  inhibition  of  the  tonus  of  skeletal  muscles. 
Reflex  inhibition  of  the  knee-jerk.  Time-relations  and  other 
characters  of  reflex  inhibition  as  exemplified  by  the  flexion- 
reflex.  Other  examples  of  inhibition  as  part  of  reflex  recipro- 
cal innervation.  The  seat  of  this  reflex  inhibition  is  intraspinal. 
Conversion  of  reflex  inhibition  into  reflex  excitation  by  strych- 
nine and  by  tetanus  toxin.  Significance  of  the  **'  central " 
situation  of  reflex  inhibition  in  the  cases  here  dealt  with. 


LECTURE    IV 

INTERACTION  BETWEEN  REFLEXES 114 

Argument:  The  "simple  reflex"  a  convenient  but  artificial  ab- 
straction. Compounding  of  reflexes.  The  principle  of  the 
common  path.  Relative  aperiodicity  of  the  final  common  path. 
Afferent  arcs  which  use  the  same  final  common  path  to  dif- 
ferent effect  have  successive  but  not  simultaneous  use  of  it 
*•'  Allied "  reflexes.  Allied  reflexes  act  harmoniously,  are 
capable  of  simultaneous  combination,  and  in  many  cases 
reinforce  one  another's  action  on  the  final  common  path. 
"  Antagonistic  "  reflexes.  AlHance  or  coalition  occurs  be- 
tween (i)  individual  reflexes  belonging  to  the  same  "type- 
reflex,"  (2)  certain  reflexes  originated  by  receptors  of  different 
species  but  situate  in  the  same  region  of  surface,  (3)  certain 
reflexes  belonging  to  proprio-ceptive  organs  secondarily  ex- 
cited by  reflexes  initiated  at  the  body-surface  (the  three 
fields  of  reception,  extero-ceptive,  intero-ceptive,  and  proprio- 
ceptive), (4)  certain  reflexes  initiated  from  widely  separate  but 
functionally  interconnected  body-regions.  Alliance  between 
reflexes  exemplified  in  inhibitory  actions  as  well  as  in  excitatory. 
Antagonistic  reflexes  interfere,  one  reflex  deferring,  intemipt- 
ing,  or  cutting  short  another,  or  precluding  the  latter  altogether 
from  taking  effect  on  the  final  common  path.     Intraspinal 


CONTENTS  xiii 

Page 
seat  of  the  interference.  Compound  reflexes  may  interfere  in 
part.  The  place  (  ?  synapse)  where  convergent  afferent  paths 
impinge  on  a  common  path  constitutes  a  mechanism  of  co- 
ordination. The  convergence  of  afferent  paths  to  form  com- 
mon paths  occurs  with  great  frequency  in  the  central  nervous 
system.  A  question  whether  any  reflexes  are  in  the  intact 
organism  wholly  neutral  one  to  another. 

LECTURE   V 

COMPOUND    REFLEXES:    SIMULTANEOUS   COMBINA- 
TION       150 

Argument:  Combination  of  reflexes  simultaneously  proceeding. 
Spread  of  reflex-response  about  a  focus.  Gray  matter  and 
lines  of  reflex  resistance.  "  Short  "  reflexes  and  "  long  "  re- 
flexes. Rules  decipherable  in  the  spread  of  reflex  reaction. 
Pfliiger's  "laws"  of  spinal  irradiation.  The  "reflex  figure." 
Variability  of  reflex  result.  Irradiation  of  a  reflex  attaches 
itself  to  the  problem  of  the  simultaneous  combination  of 
reflexes.  Co-ordination  of  reflex  result  obtains  even  when 
large  mixed  afferent  nerve-trunks  are  stimulated.  The  move- 
ment excited  by  stimulation  of  the  motor  spinal  nerve-root 
does  not  really  resemble  a  movement  evoked  reflexly  or  by 
the  will.  Extent  of  simultaneous  combinations  of  reflexes. 
Simultaneous  stimuli  arrange  themselves  naturally  in  constel- 
lations in  which  some  component  is  usually  of  pre-eminent 
intensity.  The  resulting  compound  reaction  has  both  positive 
and  negative  sides. 

LECTURE   VI 

COMPOUND  REFLEXES:    SUCCESSIVE  COMBINATION     181 

Argument:  Co-ordination  of  reflex  sequences.  Chain-reflexes 
(Loeb).  Overlapping  of  successive  stimuli  in  time.  The 
sequence  of  allied  reflexes.  Spread  of  bahnung,  '*  immediate 
induction."  Sequence  of  antagonistic  reflexes.  The  role  of 
inhibition  in  this  transition.  Views  of  the  nature  of  inhibition  : 
Rosenthal,  Wundt,  E.  Hering,  Gaskell,  Verworn,  J.  S.  Mac- 
donald.  The  "  interference  "  of  reflexes.  "  Alternating  re- 
flexes." W.  Macdougall's  view  of  "drainage  of  energy." 
"  Compensatory  reflexes."  Factors  determining  the  issue  of 
the  competition  between  antagonistic  reflexes.  "  Successive 
induction."  Rebound-effects  in  spinal  reactions;  tend  to 
restore  reflex  equilibrium.  Fatigue  in  reflexes.  Relative  high 
resistance  to  fatigue  possessed  by  the  final  common  path, 
/.  e.  motor  neurone.    Intensity  of  reaction  a  decisive  factor  in 


xiv  CONTENTS 

Page 
the  competition  of  afferent  arcs  fpr  possession  of  the  final 
common  path.  Noci-ceptive  nerves.  Prepotency  of  reflexes 
generated  by  receptors  that  considered  as  sense  organs  initiate 
sensations  with  strong  affective  tone.  Resistance  of  tonic 
reflexes  to  fatigue.  All  these  factors  render  the  conductive 
pattern  of  the  central  nervous  system  mutable  between  certain 
limits. 

LECTURE   VII 

REFLEXES  AS   ADAPTED   REACTIONS 235 

Argufne?it :  Reflexes  as  adapted  reactions.  The  purposes  of 
various  type-reflexes.  Shock  a  difficulty  in  deciphering  the 
purpose  of  reflexes.  Characters  of  spinal  shock.  Its  inci- 
dence confined  to  the  aboral  side  of  the  transection.  Its 
difference  in  severity  in  different  reflexes  and  in  different 
animals.  Shock  referable  not  to  the  irritation  of  the  trauma 
but  to  the  cutting  off  by  the  trauma  of  some  supra-spinal 
influence. 

Pseudaffective  reflexes  afford  opportunity  for  determining 
the  pain-path  in  the  spinal  cord.  This  ascends  both  lateral 
columns,  chiefly  the  one  crossed  from  side  of  stimulation. 
The  "  chloroform  cry  "  in  decerebrate  animals.  Mimesis  of 
pleasure  as  compared  with  mimesis  of  pain.  The  bodily  res- 
onance of  the  emotions.  The  theory  of  James,  Lange,  and 
Sergi.  Emotional  expressions  in  dogs  deprived  of  visceral 
and  largely  of  bodily  sensation. 


LECTURE    VIII 

SOME     ASPECTS     OF     THE     REACTIONS     OF     THE 

MOTOR   CORTEX 269 

Argument:  Remarkable  that  electrical  stimuli  applied  to  the 
organ  of  mentality  yield  with  regularity  certain  localized 
movements  from  certain  restricted  areas  of  its  surface.  Func- 
tional topography  of  "  motor "  cortex  in  the  chimpanzee, 
orang-utan,  and  gorilla.  The  cerebral  fissures,  not  functional 
boundaries.  The  anthropoid  ape  has  a  direct  pyramidal  tract 
like  that  of  man.  Recovery  of  function  not  due  to  symmetrical 
part  of  opposite  hemisphere  taking  on  supplemental  work. 
Inhibition  as  elicitable  from  the  cortex.  Reciprocal  innervation 
of  antagonistic  eye-muscles.  Reciprocal  inhibition  in  other 
muscular  groups.  Seat  of  the  inhibition  subcortical  in  these 
cases.  Reciprocal  innervation  in  willed  movements.  Pre- 
ponderant representation  in  the  "  motor  "  cortex  of  the  same 
movements  as  are  preponderantly  elicitable  as  local  reflexes 


k 


CONTENTS  XV 

Page 


om  the  cord  and  bulb.  Scanty  representation  of  certain 
movements  as  cortical  and  local  spinal  reactions  alike.  Ap- 
pearance under  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  of  movements 
reversing  the  normal  direction  of  the  preponderance.  This 
due  to  these  agents  transmuting  reciprocal  inhibition  into 
excitation.  Decerebrate  rigidity,  A  system  of  tonic  innerva- 
tion in  action.  Strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  augment  this 
innervation.  Hughlings  Jackson's  '*  co-operative  antagonism  " 
of  paired  systems  of  innervation,  one  tonic,  the  other  phasic. 
Decerebrate  rigidity  and  hemiplegic  rigidity.  The  relation  of 
the  cortex  to  receptor  organs  ;  the  pre-eminent  representation 
in  it  of  the  "  distance-receptors." 


LECTURE    IX  ^ 

1 
THE    PHYSIOLOGICAL   POSITION   AND    DOMINANCE 

OF   THE   BRAIN 308 

Argument:  The  primitive  reflex  arc.     The  diffuse  nervous  sys-  ^ 

tem  and  the  gray-centred  nervous  system  ;  the  central  nervous  ] 

system  a  part  of  the  latter.     Nervous  integration  of  the  seg-  j 

ment.     The  three  receptive  fields.     Richness  of  the  extero-  \ 

ceptive  field.     Special  refinements  of  the  receptor-organs  of  1 

the   "leading"   segments.      The   refined    receptors    of   the  | 

leading    segments    are     "distance-receptors."     "Distance-  ') 

receptors "  ;  the  projicience  of  sensations.     Extensive  inter-  j 

nuncial  paths  belonging  to  "  distance-receptors."     "  Distance-  \ 
receptors  "    initiate    precurrent    reactions.      Consummatory 
reactions ;  strong  affective  tone  of  the  sensations  adjunct  to 

them.     Receptive   range  and  locomotion.     The "  head  "as  j 

physiologically  conceived.     Proprio-ceptive  arcs  excited  sec-  I 

ondarily  to  other  arcs.     Close  functional  connection  between  \ 

the  centripetal  impulses  from  muscles  and  from  the  labyrinth.  I 
Tonic  reflexes  (of  posture,  etc.)  and  compensatory  reflexes 

are  characteristic  reactions  of  this  combined  system.     Nervous  \ 

integration  of  the  segmental  series.     Restriction  of  segmental  I 

distribution  a  factor  in  bodily  integration.     The  cerebellum  _  { 

is  the  main  ganglion  of  the   proprio-ceptive   system.     The  \ 

cerebrum  is  the  ganglion  of  the  "  distance-receptors."  1 

J 

LECTURE   X  1 

SENSUAL   FUSION 354 

Argument:  Nervous  integration   in  relation  to  bodily  movement  j 

and  to  sensation  compared.     Sensual  fusion  in  a  relatively  \ 
simple  instance  of  binocular  vision.     The  rotating  binocular 

lantern.  Flicker  sensations  generated  at  "corresponding  retinal  \ 


xvi  CONTENTS 

Page 
points  "  ;  absence  of  evidence  of  their  summation  or  interfer- 
ence either  with  synchronous  or  asynchronous  flicker  of  similar 
frequency.  Their  interference  when  the  flicker  is  of  dissimilar 
frequency.  Talbot's  law  not  applicable  to  "corresponding 
points.'*  Fechner's  paradox.  Prevalence  of  contours  under 
Weber's  law  and  under  binocular  summation  compared.  The 
physiological  initial  stages  of  the  reaction  generated  in  either 
of  a  pair  of  corresponding  retinal  points  proceeds  without 
touching  the  apparatus  of  the  twin  point.  Only  after  the  sen- 
sations initiated  from  the  right  and  left  "  points  "  have  been 
elaborated  so  far  as  to  be  well  amenable  to  introspection  does 
interference  between  the  reactions  of  the  two  (right  and  left) 
eye-systems  occur.  The  convergence  of  nerve-paths  from  the 
right  and  left  retinae  respectively  toward  one  cerebral  region 
is  significant  of  union  for  co-ordination  of  motor  reaction 
rather  than  for  synthesis  of  sensation.  Resemblances  between 
motor  and  sensual  reactions.  The  cerebrum  pre-eminently 
the  organ  of  and  for  the  adaptation  of  reactions. 


BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 395 

INDEX 403 


THE  INTEGRATIVE  ACTION  OF  THE 
NERVOUS  SYSTEM 


THE    INTEGRATIVE   ACTION   OF 
THE   NERVOUS   SYSTEM 

LECTURE    I 

INTRODUCTORY  — CO-ORDINATION   OF  THE   SIMPLE 

REFLEX 

Argument:  The  nervous  system  and  the  integration  of  bodily  reactions. 
Characteristics  of  integration  by  nervous  agency.  The  unit  mechan' 
ism  in  integration  by  the  nervous  system  is  the  reflex.  Co-ordination 
of  reflexes  one  with  another.  Co-ordination  in  the  simple  reflex. 
Conduction  in  the  reflex-arc.  Function  of  the  receptor  to  lower  for 
its  reflex-arc  the  threshold  value  of  one  kind  of  stimulus  and  to 
heighten  the  threshold  value  of  all  other  kinds  of  stimuli  for  that 
arc:  it  thus  confers  selective  excitabiHty  on  the  arc.  Differences 
between  conduction  in  nerve-trunks  and  in  reflex-arcs  respectively. 
These  probably  largely  referable  to  the  intercalation  of  synaptic  mem- 
branes in  the  conductive  mechanism  of  the  arc.  Latent  time  of 
reflexes.  Reflex  latency  inversely  proportional  to  intensity  of  stimu- 
lation. Latency  of  initial  and  incremental  reflexes.  None  of  the 
latent  interval  consumed  in  establishing  connection  between  the 
elements  of  a  resting  arc.  After- discharge  a  characteristic  of  reflex  re- 
actions. Increase  of  after-discharge  by  intensification  of  the  stimulus, 
or  by  prolongation  of  short  stimuli.  "  Inertia  "  and  "  momentum  " 
of  reflex-arc  reactions. 

Nowhere  in  physiology  does  the  cell-theory  reveal  its  presence 
more  frequently  in  the  very  framework  of  the  argument  than  at 
the  present  time  in  the  study  of  nervous  reactions.  The  cell- 
theory  at  its  inception  depended  for  exemplification  largely  on 
merely  morphological  observations ;  just  as  these  formed  origi- 
nally the  almost  exclusive  texts  for  the  Darwinian  doctrine  of 
evolution.  But  with  the  progress  of  natural  knowledge,  biology 
has  passed  beyond  the  confines  of  the  study  of  merely  visible 
form,  and  is  turning  more  and  more  to  the  subtler  and  deeper 
sciences  that  are  branches  of  energetics.  The  cell-theory  and 
the  doctrine  of  evolution  find  their  scope  more  and  more,  there- 
fore, in  the  problems  of  function,  and  have  become  more  and 


2  INTRODUCTORY  [Lect. 

more  identified  with  the  aims  and  incorporated  among  the 
methods  of  physiology. 

The  physiology  of  nervous  reactions  can  be  studied  from 
three  main  points  of  view. 

In  the  first  place,  nerve-cells,  like  all  other  cells,  lead  indi- 
vidual lives,  —  they  breathe,  they  assimilate,  they  dispense  their 
own  stores  of  energy,  they  repair  their  own  substantial  waste ; 
each  is,  in  short,  a  living  unit,  with  its  nutrition  more  or  less 
centred  in  itself.  Here,  then,  problems  of  nutrition,  regarding 
each  nerve-cell  and  regarding  the  nervous  system  as  a  whole, 
arise  comparable  with  those  presented  by  all  other  Hving  cells. 
Although  no  doubt  partly  special  to  this  specially  differentiated 
form  of  cell-life,  these  problems  are  in  general  accessible  to  the 
same  methods  as  apply  to  the  study  of  nutrition  in  other  cells  and 
tis^sues  and  in  the  body  as  a  whole.  We  owe  recently  to  Verworn 
and  his  co-workers  advances  specially  valuable  in  this  field. 

Secondly,  nervous  cells  present  a  feature  so  characteristically 
developed  in  them  as  to  be  specially  theirs.  They  have  in  ex- 
ceptional measure  the  power  to  spatially  transmit  (conduct) 
states  of  excitement  (nerve-impulses)  generated  within  them. 
Since  this  seems  the  eminent  functional  feature  of  nerve-cells 
wherever  they  exist,  its  intimate  nature  is  a  problem  co-extensive 
with  the  existence  of  nerve-cells,  and  enters  into  every  question 
regarding  the  specific  reactions  of  the  nervous  system.  This 
field  of  study  may  be  termed  that  of  nerve-cell  conduction. 

But  a  third  aspect  which  nervous  reactions  offer  to  the  physi- 
ologist is  the  integrative.  In  the  multicellular  animal,  especially 
for  those  higher  reactions  which  constitute  its  behaviour  as  a 
social  unit  in  the  natural  economy,  it  is  nervous  reaction  which 
par  excelleiice  integrates  it,  welds  it  together  from  its  compo- 
nents, and  constitutes  it  from  a  mere  collection  of  organs  an 
animal  individual.  This  integrative  action  in  virtue  of  which  the 
nervous  system  unifies  from  separate  organs  an  animal  possess- 
ing solidarity,  an  individual,  is  the  problem  before  us  in  these 
lectures.  Though  much  in  need  of  data  derived  from  the  two 
previously  mentioned  lines  of  study,  it  must  in  the  meantime  be 
carried  forward  of  itself  and  for  its  own  sake. 


I]  INTEGRATION   BY  NERVOUS  AGENCY  3 

The  integration  of  the  animal  organism  is  obviously  not  the 
result  solely  of  any  single  agency  at  work  within  it,  but  of  several. 
Thus,  there  is  the  mechanical  combination  of  the  unit  cells  of  the 
individual  into  a  single  mass.  This  is  effected  by  fibrous  stro- 
mata,  capsules  of  organs,  connective  tissue  in  general,  e.  g,  of 
the  liver,  and  indeed  the  fibrous  layer  of  the  skin  encapsulating 
the  whole  body.  In  muscles  this  mechanical  integration  of  the 
organ  may  arrive  at  providing  a  single  cord  tendon  by  which 
the  tensile  stress  of  a  myriad  contractile  cells  can  be  additively 

ncentrated  upon  a  single  place  of  application. 
Integration  also  results  from  chemical  agency.  Thus,  repro- 
ductive organs,  remote  one  from  another,  are  given  solidarity 
as  a  system  by  communication  that  is  of  chemical  quality; 
lactation  supervenes  post  pat  turn  in  all  the  mammary  glands  of 
a  bitch  subsequent  to  thoracic  transection  of  the  spinal  cord 
severing  all  nervous  communication  between  the  pectoral  and 
the  inguinal  mammae  (Goltz).  In  digestive  organs  we  find 
chemical  agency  co-ordinating  the  action  of  separate   glands, 

d  thus  contributing  to  the  solidarity  of  function  of  the  diges- 
ive  glands  as  a  whole.  The  products  of  salivary  digestion  on 
reaching  the  pyloric  region  of  the  stomach,  and  the  gastric 
secretion  on  reaching  the  mucosa  of  the  duodenum,  make  there 
substances  which  absorbed  duly  excite  heightened  secretion  of 
gastric  and  of  pancreatic  juice  respectively  suited  to  continue 
the  digestion  of  the  substances  initiating  the  reaction  (Bayliss  and 
Starling,  Edkins).  Again,  there  is  the  integrating  action  effected 
by  the  circulation  of  the  blood.  The  gaseous  exchanges  at  one 
limited  surface  of  the  body  are  made  serviceable  for  the  life 
of  every  living  unit  in  the  body.  By  the  blood  the  excess  o1 
heat  produced  in  one  set  of  organs  is  brought  to  redress  the 
loss  of  heat  in  others;    and  so  on. 

But  the  integrative  action  of  the  nervous  system  is  different 
from  these,  in  that  its  agent  is  not  mere  intercellular  material, 
as  in  connective  tissue,  nor  the  transference  of  material  in  mass, 
as  by  the  circulation ;  it  works  through  living  lines  of  stationary 
cells  along  which  it  despatches  waves  of  physico-chemical  dis- 
turbance, and  these  act  as  releasing  forces  in  distant  organs 


cni 

^ 

tiv 


4  INTRODUCTORY  [Lect. 

where  they  finally  impinge.  Hence  it  is  not  surprising  that 
nervous  integration  has  the  feature  of  relatively  high  speedy  a 
feature  peculiarly  distinctive  of  integrative  correlation  in  animals 
as  contrasted  with  that  of  plants,  the  latter  having  no  nervous 
system  in  the  ordinary  sense  of  the  word. 

The  nervous  system  is  in  a  certain  sense  the  highest  expres- 
sion of  that  which  French  physiologists  term  the  milieu  interne. 
With  the  transition  from  the  unicellular  organism  to  the  multi- 
cellular a  new  element  enters  general  physiology.  The  phe- 
nomena of  general  physiology  in  the  unicellular  organism  can 
be  divided  into  two  great  groups ;  namely,  those  occurring  within 
the  cell,  intracellular,  and  those  occurring  at  the  surface  of  the 
cell,  in  which  forces  that  are  associated  with  surfaces  of  separa- 
tion have  opportunity  for  play  at  the  boundary  between  the 
organism  and  its  environment  But  in  the  multicellular  organ- 
ism a  third  great  group  of  phenomena  exists  in  addition  ;  namely, 
those  which  are  interceWulsLV,  occurring  in  that  complex  material 
which  the  organism  deposits  in  quantity  in  the  intercellular  inter- 
stices of  its  mass  as  a  connecting  medium  between  its  individual 
living  units. 

When  the  intercellular  substance  is  solid,  e»  g.  in  many  con- 
nective tissues,  the  physiological  agencies  for  which  it  afifords  a 
field  of  operation  are  mechanical  rather  than  chemical.  The 
organism  obtains  from  it  scaffolding  for  supporting  its  weight, 
levers  for  application  of  its  forces,  etc.,  and  in  this  degree  the 
intercellular  material  performs  an  integrative  function.  Where 
the  intercellular  material  is  fluid,  as  in  blood,  lymph,  and  tissue 
juice,  it  constitutes  a  field  of  operation  for  agencies  chemical 
rather  than  mechanical.  The  intricacy  of  the  chemistry  of  this 
milieu  interne  is  shown  by  nothing  better  than  by  the  specificity 
of  the  precipitins,  etc.,  the  intercellular  media  for  each  separate 
animal  species  yielding  its  own  particular  kinds.  The  cells  of  a 
multicellular  organism  have  therefore  in  addition  to  an  environ- 
mental medium  in  which  the  organism  as  a  whole  is  bathed,  and 
to  which  they  react  either  directly  or  through  the  medium  of 
surface  cells,  an  internal  medium  created  by  their  organism  itself, 
and  in  many  respects  specific  to  itself. 


I]    RECEPTION,  CONDUCTION,  AND  END-EFFECT     5 

But  the  internal  interconnection  of  the  multicellular  organism 
is  not  restricted  to  intercellular  material.  Intercellular  material 
is,  after  all,  no  living  channel  of  communication,  delicately  re- 
sponsive to  living  changes  though  it  may  be.  An  actually  living 
internal  bond  is  developed.  When  the  animal  body  reaches  some 
degree  of  multicellular  complexity,  special  cells  assume  the  ex- 
press office  of  connecting  together  other  cells.  Such  cells,  since 
their  function  is  to  stretch  from  one  cell  to  another,  are  usually 
elongated ;  they  form  protoplasmic  threads  and  they  intercon- 
nect by  conducting  nervous  impulses.  And  we  find  this  living 
bond  the  one  employed  where,  as  said  above,  speed  and  nicety 
of  time  adjustment  are  required,  as  in  animal  movements,  and 
also  where  nicety  of  spatial  adjustment  is  essential,  as  also  in 
animal  movements.  It  is  in  view  of  this  interconnecting  func- 
tion of  the  nervous  system  that  that  field  of  study  of  nervous 
reactions  which  was  called  at  the  outset  the  third  or  integrative, 
assumes  its  due  importance.  The  due  activity  of  the  intercon- 
nection resolves  itself  into  the  co-ordination  of  the  parts  of  the 
animal  mechanism  by  reflex  action. 

It  is  necessary  to  be  clear  as  to  what  we  understand  by  the 
expression  "  reflex  "  action. 

In  plants  and  animals  occur  a  number  of  actions  the  initia- 
tion of  which  is  traceable  to  events  in  their  environment.  fThe 
event  in  the  environment  is  some  change  which  acts  on  the  organ- 
ism as  an  exciting  stimulus,  \The  energy  which  is  imparted  to 
the  organism  by  the  stimulus  is  often  far  less  in  quantity  than 
the  energy  which  the  organism  itself  sets  free  in  the  movement 
or  other  effect  which  it  exhibits  in  consequence  of  the  applica- 
tion of  the  stimulus.  This  excess  of  energy  must  be  referred  to 
energy  potential  in  the  organism  itself.  The  change  in  the 
environment  evidently  acts  as  a  releasing  force  upon  the  living 
machinery  of  the  organism.  The  source  of  energy  set  free  is 
traced  to  chemical  compounds  in  the  organism.  These  are  of 
high  potential  value,  and  in  immediate  or  mediate  consequence 
of  the  stimulus  decompose  partly,  and  so  liberate  external  from 
internal  energy.  It  is  perfectly  conceivable,  and  in  many  undif- 
ferentiated organisms,  especially  in  unicellular,  e.  g.  amoeba,  is 


6  INTRODUCTORY  [Lect. 

actually  the  case,  that  one  and  the  same  living  structure  not 
only  undergoes  this  physico-chemical  change  at  the  point  at 
which  an  external  agent  is  applied,  but  is  subject  to  spread  of 
that  change  from  particle  to  particle  along  it,  so  that  there  then 
ensue  in  it  changes  of  form,  movement  1-  In  such  a  case  the 
initial  reaction  or  reception  of  the  stimulus,  the  spatial  transmis- 
sion or  conduction  of  the  reaction,  and  the  motor  or  other  eftd- 
effect,  are  all  processes  that  occur  in  one  and  the  same  living 
structure.]  But  in  many  organisms  these  separable  parts  of  the 
reaction  are  exhibited  by  separate  and  specific  structures.  Sup- 
pose an  animal  turn  its  head  in  response  to  a  sudden  light 
Large  fields  of  its  body  take  part  in  the  reaction,  but  also  large 
fields  of  it  do  not.  Some  of  its  musculature  contracts,  particu- 
larly certain  pieces  of  its  skeletal  musculature.  The  external 
stimulus  is,  so  to  say,  led  to  them  by  certain  nerves  in  the 
altered  form  of  a  nervous  impulse.  If  the  neck  nerves  are 
severed  the  end-effect  is  cut  out  of  part  of  the  field ;  and  the 
nerves  themselves  cannot  exhibit  movement  on  application  of 
the  stimulus.  The  optic  nerve  itself  is  unable  to  enter  into  a 
heightened  phase  of  its  own  specific  activity  on  the  application 
of  light  Initiation  of  nervous  activity  by  light  is  the  exclusive 
(in  this  instance)  function  of  cells  in  the  retina,  retinal  receptors. 
In  such  cases  there  exist  three  separable  structures  for  the  three 
gTQcesses  —  initiation,  conduction,  and  end-effect. 

These  reactions,  in  which  there  follows  on  an  initiating  reac- 
tion an  end-effect  reached  through  the  mediation  of  a  conductor, 
itself  incapable  either  of  the  end-effect  or,  under  natural  condi- 
tions, of  the  inception  of  the  reaction,  are  "  reflexes."  The  con- 
ductors  are  nerve./  Usually  the  spaces  and  times  bridged  across 
by  the  conductors  are  quite  large,  and  easily  capable  of  measure- 
ment Now  there  occur  cases,  especially  within  the  unicellular 
organism  and  the  unicellular  organ,  where  the  spaces  and  times 
bridged  are  minute.  In  them  spread  of  response  may  involve 
"  conduction "  (Poteriodendron,  Vorticella)  in  some  degree 
specific.  Yet  to  cases  where  neither  histologically  nor  physi- 
ologically a  specific  conductor  can  be  detected,  it  seems  better 
not  to  apply  the  term  '*  reflex."     It  seems  better  to  reserve  that 


I]  CO-ORDINATION   OF   REFLEXES  7 

expression  for  reactions  employing  specifically  recognizable 
nerve-processes  and  morphologically  differentiated  nerve-cells ; 
the  more  so  because  the  process  of  conduction  in  nerve  is 
probably  a  specialized  one,  in  which  the  qualities  of  speed  and 
freedom  from  inertia  of  reaction  have  been  attained  to  a  degree 
not  reached  elsewhere  since  not  elsewhere  demanded. 

The  conception  of  a  reflex  therefore  embraces  that  of  at 
least  three  separable  structures,  —  an  effector  organ,  e.  g.y  gland 
cells  or  muscle  cells;  a  conducting  nervous  path  or  conductor 
leading  to  that  organ ;  and  an  initiating  organ  or  receptor  whence 
the  reaction  starts.  The  conductor  consists,  in  the  reactions 
which  we  have  to  study,  of  at  least  two  nerve-cells,  —  one  con- 
nected with  the  receptor,  the  other  with  the  effector.  For  our 
purpose  ftRe  receptor  is  best  included  as  a  part  of  the  nervous 
system,  and  so  it  is  convenient  to  speak  of  the  whole  chain  of 
structures  —  receptor,  conductor,  and  effector  —  as  a  reflex-arc/^ 
All  that  part  of  the  chain  which  leads  up  to  but  does  not  include 
the  effector  and  the  nerve-cell  attached  to  this  latter,  is  conven- 
iently distinguished  as  the  afferent-arc,  ^^^^^^^^ 

The  reflex-arc  is  the  unit  mechanism  of  the  nervous  system 
when  that  system  is  regarded  in  its  integrative  function.  The 
unit  reaction  in  nervous  integration  is  the  refleXy  because  every 
reflex  is  an  integrative  reaction  and  no  nervous  action  short  of  a 
reflex  is  a  complete  act  of  integration.  The  nervous  synthesis 
of  an  individual  from  what  without  it  were  a  mere  aggregation 
of  commensal  organs  resolves  itself  into  co-ordination  by  reflex 
action.  But  though  the  unit  reaction  in  the  integration  is  a 
reflex,  not  every  reflex  is  a  unit  reaction,  since  some  reflexes 
are  compounded  of  simpler  reflexes.  Co-ordination,  therefore, 
is  in  part  the  compounding  of  reflexes.  In  this  co-ordination  ) 
there  are  therefore  obviously  two  grades.  --J 

The  simple  reflex.  There  is  the  co-ordination  which  a  reflex 
action  introduces  when  it  makes  an  effector  organ  responsive  to 
excitement  of  a  receptor,  all  other  parts  of  the  organism  being 
supposed  indifferent  to  and  indifferent  for  that  reaction.  In  this 
grade  of  co-ordination  the  reflex  is  taken  apart,  as  if  separable 
from   all  other  reflex  actions.     This  is  the  simple   reflex.     A 


8  THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

simple  reflex  is  probably  a  purely  abstract  conception,  because 
all  parts  of  the  nervous  system  are  connected  together  and  no 
part  of  it  is  probably  ever  capable  of  reaction  without  affecting 
and  being  affected  by  various  other  parts,  and  it  is  a  system 
certainly  never  absolutely  at  rest.  But  the  simple  reflex  is  a 
convenient,  if  not  a  probable,  fiction.  Reflexes  are  of  various 
degrees  of  complexity,  and  it  is  helpful  in  analyzing  complex 
reflexes  to  separate  from  them  reflex  components  which  we 
may  consider  apart  and  therefore  treat  as  though  they  were 
simple  reflexes. 

In  the  simple  reflex  there  is  exhibited  the  first  grade  of  co- 
ordination. But  it  is  obvious  that  if  the  integration  of  the 
animal  mechanism  is  due  to  co-ordination  by  reflex  action, 
reflex  actions  must  themselves  be  co-ordinated  one  with  another ; 
for  co-ordination  by  reflex  action  there  must  be  co-ordination  of 
reflex  actions.  This  latter  is  the  second  grade  of  co-ordination. 
The  outcome  of  the  normal  reflex  action  of  the  organism  is  an 
orderly  coadjustment  and  sequence  of  reactions.  This  is  very 
patently  expressed  by  the  skeletal  musculature.  The  co-ordina- 
tion involves  orderly  coadjustment  of  a  number  of  simple  reflexes 
occurring  simultaneously^  i.  e,  a  reflex  pattern,  figure,  or  "  com- 
plication," if  one  may  warp  a  psychological  term  for  this  use ; 
orderly  succession  involves  due  supercession  of  one  reflex  by 
another,  or  of  one  group  of  reflexes  by  another  group,  /.  e, 
orderly  change  from  one  reflex  pattern  or  figure  to  another. 
For  this  succession  to  occur  in  an  orderly  manner  no  com- 
ponent of  the  previous  reflex  may  remain  which  would  be  out 
of  harmony  with  the  new  reflex  that  sets  in.  When  the  change 
from  one  reflex  to  another  occurs  it  is  therefore  usually  a  far- 
reaching  change  spread  over  a  wide  range  of  nervous  arcs. 

This  compounding  of  reflexes  with  orderliness  of  coadjust- 
ment and  of  sequence  constitutes  co-ordination,  and  want  of  it 
inco-ordination.  We  may  therefore  in  regard  to  co-ordination 
distinguish  co-ordination  of  reflexes  simultaneously  proceeding, 
and  co-ordination  of  reflexes  successively  proceeding.  The 
main  secret  of  nervous  co-ordination  lies  evidently  in  the 
compounding  of  reflexes. 


I]  THE   RECEPTOR  9 

Co-ordination  in  the  simple  reflex.  It  is  best  to  clear  the 
way  toward  the  more  complex  problems  of  co-ordination  by 
considering  as  an  earlier  step  that  which  was  termed  above, 
the  first  grade  of  co-ordination,  or  that  of  the  simple  reflex. 
From  the  point  of  view  of  its  office  as  integrator  of  the 
animal  mechanism,  the  whole  function  of  the  nervous  system 
can  be  summed  up  in  the  one  word,  conduction*  In  the  simple 
reflex  the  evidence  of  co-ordination  is  that  the  outcome  of 
the  reflex  as  expressed  by  the  activity  induced  in  the  effector 
organ  is  a  response  appropriate  to  the  stimulus  imparted  to  the 
receptor.  This  due  propriety  of  end-effect  is  largely  traceable 
to  the  action  of  the  conductor  mediating  between  receptor  and 
affector.  Knowledge  of  the  features  of  this  "  conduction  "  is 
therefore  a  prime  object  of  study  in  this  connection. 

But  we  have  first  to  remember  that  in  dealing  with  reflexes 
even  experimentally  we  very  usually  deal  with  them  as  reactions 
for  which  the  reflex-arc  as  a  whole  and  without  any  separation 
into  constituent  parts  is  laid  under  contribution.  (The  reflex- 
arc  thus  taken  includes  the  receptor.  It  is  assuredly  as  truly  a 
functional  part  of  the  arc  as  any  other.  J  But,  for  analysis  of  the 
arc's  conduction,  it  is  obvious  that  by  including  the  receptor  we 
are  including  a  structure  which,  as  its  name  implies,  adaptation 
has  specialized  for  excitation  of  a  kind  different  from  that 
obtaining  for  all  the  rest  of  the  arc.  It  is  therefore  advanta- 
geous, as  we  have  to  include  the  receptor  in  the  reflex-arc,  to 
consider  what  characters  its  inclusion  probably  grafts  upon  the 
functioning  of  the  arc. 

Marshall  Hall  ^3  *  drew  attention  to  the  greater  ease  with 
which  reflexes  can  be  elicited  from  receptive  surfaces  than  from 
afferent  nerve-trunks  themselves ;  and  this  has  often  been  con- 
firmed (Eckhard,  Biedermann).  Steinach  ^^  has  measured  the 
lowering  of  the  threshold  value  of  stimulation  when  in  the  frog 
a  reflex  is  elicited  by  a  mechanical  stimulus  applied  to  skin 
instead  of  to  cutaneous  afferent  nerve.  The  lowering  is  con- 
siderable. There  are  numerous  instances  in  which  particular 
reflexes  can  be  elicited  from  the  receptive  surface  by  particular 

*  The  reference  numbers  in  the  text  refer  to  the  bibliographical  list  at  the 
end  of  the  volume. 


lo  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

stimuli  only.  Goltz*^  endeavoured  in  vain  to  evoke  the  reflex 
croak  of  the  female  frog  by  applying  to  the  skin  electrical  stim- 
uli. Mechanical  stimuli  of  non-nocuous  kind  were  the  only  stim- 
uli that  proved  efifective.  From  the  afferent  nerve  itself  by  direct 
stimulation  the  reflex  could  but  rarely  be  elicited  at  all.  Later 
Goltz's  pupil  Gergens  ^^  succeeded  in  provoking  the  reflex  by  ap- 
plying to  the  skin  a  mild  discharge  from  an  influence  machine. 

A  remarkable  reflex  ^^  is  obtainable  from  the  planta  of  the 
hind  foot  in  the  "  spinal "  dog.  The  movement  provoked  is  a 
brief  strong  extension  at  knee,  hip,  and  ankle.  This  is  the 
"  extensor-thrust."  It  seems  obtainable  only  by  a  particular 
kind  of  mechanical  stimulation.  I  have  never  succeeded  in 
eliciting  it  by  any  form  of  electrical  stimulation,  nor  by  any 
stimulation  applied  directly  to  an  afferent  nerve-trunk. 

Again,  a  very  characteristic  reflex  in  the  cat  is  the  pinna- 
reflex.^  If  the  tip  of  the  pinna  be  squeezed,  or  tickled,  or  in 
some  cases  even  touched,  the  pinna  itself  is  crumpled  so  that 
its  free  end  is  turned  backward,  as  in  Darwin's  ^  picture  of  a 
cat  prepared  to  attack.  The  afferent  nerve  of  this  reflex  appears 
to  be  in  part  at  least  not  the  cranial  fifth  nerve,  but  the  foremost 
cervical.  The  reflex  emerges  very  early  from  the  shock  of  de- 
cerebration  and  is  submerged  very  late  in  chloroform  narcosis. 
This  reflex,  easily  elicitable  as  it  is  by  various  mechanical  stimuli 
to  the  skin,  I  have  never  succeeded  in  provoking  by  any  form  of 
electrical  stimulation. 

The  same  sort  of  difference,  though  less  marked  in  degree,  is 
exhibited  by  the  scratch-reflex.^'.  ^^,  251,  252,  300  j^is  reflex  is 
one  in  which  various  forms  of  innocuous  mechanical  stimula- 
tion (rubbing,  tickling,  tapping)  applied  to  the  skin  of  the 
back  behind  the  shoulder  evoke  a  rhythmic  flexion  (scratching 
movement)  of  the  hind  limb,  the  foot  being  brought  toward 
the  seat  of  stimulation.  This  reflex  in  the  spinal  dog,  although 
usually  elicitable,  varies  much  under  various  circumstances  in  its 
degree  of  elicitability.  When  easily  elicitable  it  can  be  evoked 
by  various  forms  of  electrical  stimulation  as  well  as  by  mechan- 
ical ;  but  when  not  easily  elicitable  electrical  stimuli  altogether 
fail,  while  rubbing  and  other  suitable  mechanical  stimuli  still 
evoke  it,  though  not  so  readily  or  vigorously  as  usual. 


I] 


ADEQUATE   STIMULUS 


II 


A  question  germane  to  this  is  the  oft-debated  sensitivity  of 
various  internal  organs.  Direct  stimulation  of  various  afferent 
nerves  of  the  visceral  system  is  itself  well  known  to  yield 
reflexes  on  blood-pressure,  etc.     But  in  regard  to  the  sensitivity 


Figure  i.  —  Rise  of  arterial  pressure  produced,  in  the  cat  under  CHCl  and  curare,  after 
double  vagotomy,  by  the  rapid  injection  of  2.5  c.c.  of  saline  solution  in  the  common 
bile-duct,  i.  g.,  by  distension  of  the  duct.  Time  marked  below  in  seconds  Ccf.  Bibliogr. 
No.  194). 


12  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

of  the  organs  themselves  we  have,  on  the  one  hand,  the  passage 
of  bilestones,  renal  calculi,  etc.,  accompanied  by  intense  sensa- 
tions, and  on  the  other  hand  the  insensitivity  of  these  ducts  and 
various  allied  visceral  parts  as  noted  by  Haller^  and  observed 
by  surgeons  working  under  circumstances  favorable  for  examin- 
ing the  question.  The  stimulation  which  excites  pain  in  these 
internal  organs  is  usually  of  mechanical  kind,  e.g.  calculus,  and 
the  surgeon's  knife  and  needle  provide  mechanical  stimuli,  and 
Haller  and  his  co-workers  in  their  research  employed  multiform 
stimuli,  many  of  them  mechanical  in  quality.  But  though 
mechanical,  the  latter  are  remote  in  quality  from  the  former; 
the  former  are  distensile.  The  action  of  a  calculus  can  be  imi- 
tated by  injecting  fluid  of  itself  innocuous.  Marked  reflex 
effects  can  then  be  excited  ^^  from  the  very  organs  (Fig.  i),  the 
cutting  and  wounding  of  which  remains  without  effect.  For 
H^lkr's  and  the  surgical  experience  to  be  harmonized  with  the 
medical  evidence  from  calculi,  etc.,  all  that  is  necessary  is  that 
the  mechanical  stimulation  be  adequate^  and  to  be  adequate  it 
must  be  of  a  certain  kind.  Thus  we  see  that  when  the  mechan- 
ical stimulation  employed  resembles  that  occurring  in  the  natural 
accidents  that  concern  medicine,  the  experimental  results  fall 
into  line  with  those  observed  at  the  bedside. 

Therefore  we  may  infer  provisionally  —  for  the  facts  justify 
only  a  guarded  judgment — that  the  part  played  by  the  re- 
ceptor in  the  reflex-arc  is  in  the  main  what  from  other  evidence 
it  is  inferred  to  be  in  the  case  of  the  receptors  as  sense-ox^dSis ; 
namely,  a  mechanism  more  or  less  attuned  to  respond  specially 
to  a  certain  one  or  ones  of  the  agencies  that  act  as  stimuli  to 
the  body.  We  may  suppose  this  special  attuning  acts  as  does 
specialization  in  so  many  cases,  namely  by  rendering  more  apt 
for  a  certain  kind  of  stimulus  and  at  the  same  time  less  apt  for 
stimuli  of  other  kinds.  The  main  function  of  the  receptor  is 
therefore  ^^  to  lower  the  threshold  of  excitability  of  the  arc  for  one 
kind  of  stimulus y  and  to  heighten  it  for  all  others.  This  is  quite 
comparable  with  the  low  threshold  for  touch-sensation  under 
mechanical  stimulation  applied  to  a  hair  (v.  Frey)  ^"^  contrasted 
with  the  high  threshold  under  electrical  stimulation  of  the  skin 


I]     SELECTIVE  EXCITABILITY  OF  REFLEX-ARC      13 

(v.  Frey).  Adaptation  has  evolved  a  mechanism  for  which  one 
kind  of  stimulus  is  the  appropriate,  that  is,  the  adequate  stim- 
ulus', other  stimuli  than  the  adequate  not  being  what  the 
adaptation  fitted  the  mechanism  for,  are  at  a  disadvantage. 
Electrical  stimuli  are  in  most  cases  far  the  most  convenient  to 
use  for  experimental  work,  because  of  their  easy  control,  espe- 
cially in  regard  to  intensity  and  time.  But  electrical  stimuli  not 
being  of  common  occurrence  in  nature,  there  has  been  no  chance 
for  adaptation  to  evolve  in  the  organism  receptors  appropriate 
for  such  stimuli.  Therefore  we  may  say  that  electricity  never 
constitutes  the  adequate  stimulus  for  any  receptor,  since  it  is 
always  an  artificial  form  of  stimulus,  and  every  adequate  stimulus 
must  obviously  be  a  natural  form  of  stimulation.  It  is  therefore 
rather  a  matter  for  surprise  that  electrical  stimuli  applied  to 
receptor  organs  are  as  efficient  excitors  of  reflexes  as  they 
in  fact  prove  to  be.  It  is  particularly  in  regard  to  a  class  of 
reflexes  whose  receptive  cells  seem  attuned  specially  to  react  to 
nocuous  agents,  agents  that  threaten  to  do  local  damage,  that 
electrical  stimuli  are  found  to  be  excellently  effective.  But  the 
conditions  of  adaptation  to  stimuli  appear  here  peculiar ;  and 
there  will  be  better  opportunity  of  considering  them  later. 

We  infer,  therefore,  that  the  main  contribution  made  to  the 
mechanism  of  the  reflex-arc  by  that  part  of  it  which  constitutes 
the  receptor  is  selective  excitability.  It  thus  contributes  to 
co-ordination,  for  it  renders  its  arc  prone  to  reply  to  certain 
stimuli,  while  other  arcs  not  having  that  kind  of  receptor  do  not 
reply,  and  it  renders  its  arc  unlikely  to  reply  to  certain  other 
stimuli  to  which  other  arcs  are  likely  to  respond.  It  will  thus, 
while  providing  increase  of  responsiveness  on  the  part  of  the 
organism  to  the  environment,  tend  to  prevent  confusion  of  re- 
actions (inco-ordination)  by  Hmiting  to  particular  stimuli  a 
particular  reaction. 

On  the  whole,  we  may  regard  the  receptor  as  being  con- 
cerned with  the  mode  of  excitation  rather  than  with  the  features 
of  conduction  of  the  reflex-arc,  and  may  now  return  to  that 
conduction,  which  itself  has  important  co-ordinative  characters. 
Nervous  conduction  has  been  studied  chiefly  in  nerve-trunks. 


14  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

Conduction  in  reflexes  is  of  course  for  its  spatially  greater  part 
conduction  along  nerve-trunks,  yet  reflex  conduction  in  toto 
difl"ers  widely  from  nerve-trunk  conduction. 

Salient  among  the  characteristic  differences  between  con- 
duction in  nerve-trunks  and  in  reflex-arcs  respectively  are  the 
following : 

Conduction  in  reflex-arcs  exhibits  (i)  slower  speed  as  meas- 
ured by  the  latent  period  between  application  of  stimulus  and 
appearance  of  end-effect,  this  difference  being  greater  for  weak 
stimuli  than  for  strong;  (2)  less  close  correspondence  between 
the  moment  of  cessation  of  stimulus  and  the  moment  of  cessation 
of  end-effect,  i.e.y  there  is  a  marked  "  after-discharge;  "  (3)  less 
close  correspondence  between  rhythm  of  stimulus  and  rhythm  of 
end-effect;  (4)  less  close  correspondence  between  the  grading  of 
intensity  of  the  stimulus  and  the  grading  of  intensity  of  the  end- 
effect;  (5)  considerable  resistance  to  passage  of  a  single  nerve- 
impulse,  but  a  resistance  easily  forced  by  a  succession  of  impulses 
(temporal  summation)  ;  (6)  irreversibility  of  direction  instead  of 
reversibility  as  in  nerve-trunks ;  (7)  fatigability  in  contrast  with 
the  comparative  unfatigability  of  nerve-trunks ;  (8)  much  greater 
variability  of  the  threshold  value  of  stimulus  than  in  nerve- 
trunks  ;  (9)  refractory  period,  "  bahnung,"  inhibition,  and  shock, 
in  degrees  unknown  for  nerve-trunks;  (10)  much  greater  de- 
pendence on  blood-circulation,  oxygen  (Verworn,  Winterstein, 
v.  Baeyer,  etc.);  (11)  much  greater  susceptibility  to  various 
drugs  —  anaesthetics. 

These  differences  between  conduction  in  reflex-arcs  and  nerve- 
trunks  respectively  appear  referable  to  that  part  of  the  arc  which 
lies  in  gray  matter.  The  constituents  of  gray  matter  over  and 
above  those  which  exist  also  in  nerve-trunks  are  the  nerve-cell 
bodies  (perikarya),^'^  the  fine  nerve-cell  branches  (dendritic 
and  axonic  nerve-fibres),  and  neuroglia. 

Neuroglia  exists  in  white  matter  as  well  as  in  gray,  and  there 
is  no  good  ground  for  attributing  the  above  characteristics  of  con- 
duction in  reflex-arcs  to  that  part  of  the  arcs  which  consists  of 
white  matter.  It  is  improbable,  therefore,  on  that  ground  that  the 
features  of  the  conduction  are  due  to  neuroglia.     Indeed  there 


CONDUCTION   IN   REFLEX-ARC  15 

no  good  evidence  that  neuroglia  is  concerned  directly  in 
lervous  conduction  at  all.  As  to  perikarya  (nerve-cell  bodies) 
the  experiment  of  Bethe^"*  on  the  motor  perikarya  of  the 
ganglion  of  the  second  antenna  of  Carcinus,  and  the  experiments 
of  Steinach^^  on  the  perikarya  of  the  spinal-root  ganglion,  also 
the  observation  by  Langley  ^^^  that  nicotin  has  little  effect  when 
applied  to  the  spinal-root  ganglion,  though  breaking  conduction 
in  sympathetic  ganglia,  all  indicate  more  or  less  directly  that  it 
is  not  to  the  perikarya  that  the  characteristic  features  of  reflex- 
arc  conduction  are  referable.  Similarly,  the  experiments  of 
Exner,'^  and  of  Moore  and  Reynolds,*^^  detecting  no  delay  in 
transmission  through  the  spinal-root  ganglion,  —  though  observa- 
tions by  Wundt  *'  and  by  Gad  and  Joseph  ^^  had  a  different  result, 
—  withdraw  from  the  perikaryon  the  responsibility  for  another 
feature  characteristic  of  reflex-arc  conduction.  Again,  histolog- 
ical observations  by  Cajal,  van  Gehuchten,  and  others,  indicate 
that  in  various  cases  the  line  of  conduction  may  run  not  through 
the  perikaryon  at  all,  but  direct  from  dendrite  stem  to  axone. 

As  to  the  nerve-cell  branches  (dendrites,  axones,  and  axone- 
collaterals)  which  are  so  prominent  as  histological  characters  of 
gray  matter,  they  are  in  many  cases  perfectly  continuous  with 
nerve-fibres  outside  whose  conductive  features  are  known  by 
study  of  nerve-trunks,  and  they  also  are  themselves  nerve-fibres, 
though  smaller  in  calibre  than  those  outside.  It  seems  therefore 
scarcely  justifiable  to  suppose  that  conduction  along  nerve-fibres 
assumes  in  the  gray  matter  characters  so  widely  different  from 
those  it  possesses  elsewhere  as  to  account  for  the  dissimilarity 
between  reflex-arc  conduction  and  nerve-trunk  conduction 
respectively. 

In  this  difficulty  there  rises  forcibly  to  mind  that  not  the  least 
fruitful  of  the  facts  which  the  cell-theory  rests  upon  and  brings  to- 
gether is  the  existence  at  the  confines  of  the  cells  composing  the 
organism  of  "  surfaces  of  separation  "  between  the  adjacent  cells. 
In  certain  syncytial  cases  such  surfaces  are  not  apparent,  but  with 
most  of  the  cells  in  the  organism  their  existence  is  undisputed, 
and  they  play  an  important  r6le  in  a  great  number  of  physio- 
logical processes.     Now  in  addition  to  the  structural  elements 


i6  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

of  gray  matter  specified  above,  there  is  one  other  which  certainly 
in  many  cases  exists.  The  gray  matter  is  the  field  of  nexus 
between  neurone  and  neurone.  Except  in  sympathetic  ganglia, 
the  place  of  nexus  between  neurone  and  neurone  lies  nowhere 
else  than  in  gray  matter.  We  know  of  no  reflex-arc  composed 
of  one  single  neurone  only.  In  other  words,  every  reflex-arc 
must  contain  a  nexus  between  one  neurone  and  another.  The 
reflex-arc  must,  therefore,  on  the  cell-theory,  be  expected  to 
include  not  only  intracellular  conduction,  but  intercellular  con- 
duction. But  on  the  current  view  of  the  structure  of  the  nerve- 
fibres  of  nerve-trunks  the  conduction  observed  in  nerve-trunks 
is  entirely  and  only  intraceWulsLr  conduction.  Perhaps,  therefore, 
the  difference  between  reflex-arc  conduction  and  nerve-trunk 
conduction  is  related  to  an  additional  element  in  the  former, 
namely,  ^"«/^n:ellular  conduction.  If  there  exists  any  surface  or 
separation  at  the  nexus  between  neurone  and  neurone,  much  of 
what  is  characteristic  of  the  conduction  exhibited  by  the  reflex- 
arc  might  be  more  easily  explicable.  At  the  nexus  between 
cells  if  there  be  not  actual  confluence,  there  must  be  a  surface 
of  separation.  At  the  nexus  between  efferent  neurone  and  the 
muscle-cell,  electrical  organ,  etc.,  which  it  innervates,  it  is 
generally  admitted  that  there  is  not  actual  confluence  of  the 
two  cells  together,  but  that  a  surface  separates  them ;  and  a 
surface  of  separation  is  physically  a  membrane.  As  regards 
a  number  of  the  features  enumerated  above  as  distinguishing 
reflex-arc  conduction  from  nerve-trunk  conduction,  there  is  evi- 
dence that  similar  features^  though  not  usually  in  such  marked 
extent y  characterize  conduction  from  efferent  nerve-fibre  to  efferent 
orgatty  e.  g.y  in  nerve-muscle  preparation,  in  nerve-electric-organ 
preparation,  etc.  Here  change  in  character  of  conduction  is  not 
due  to  perikarya  (nerve-cell  bodies),  for  such  are  not  present. 
The  change  may  well  be  referable  to  the  surface  of  separation 
admittedly  existent  between  efferent  neurone  and  effector  cell. 

If  the  conductive  element  of  the  neurone  be  fluid,  and  if  at 
the  nexus  between  neurone  and  neurone  there  does  not  exist 
actual  confluence  of  the  conductive  part  of  one  cell  with  the 
conductive  part  of  the  other,  ^.^.  if  there  is  not  actual  continuity 


I]  THE   SYNAPSE  17 

of  physical  phase  between  them,  there  must  be  a  surface  of 
separation.  Even  should  a  membrane  visible  to  the  microscope 
not  appear,  the  mere  fact  of  non-confluence  of  the  one  with  the 
other  implies  the  existence  of  a  surface  of  separation.  Such 
a  surface  might  restrain  diffusion,  bank  up  osmotic  pressure, 
restrict  the  movement  of  ions,  accumulate  electric  changes, 
support  a  double  electric  layer,  alter  in  shape  and  surface- 
tension  with  changes  in  difference  of  potential,  alter  in  difference 
of  potential  with  changes  in  surface-tension  or  in  shape,  or  inter- 
vene as  a  membrane  between  dilute  solutions  of  electrolytes  of 
different  concentration  or  colloidal  suspensions  with  different 
sign  of  charge.  It  would  be  a  mechanism  where  nervous  con- 
duction, especially  if  predominantly  physical  in  nature,  might 
have  grafted  upon  it  characters  just  such  as  those  differentiating 
reflex-arc  conduction  from  nerve-trunk  conduction.  For  in-  f 
stance,  change  from  reversibility  of  direction  of  conduction  to 
irreversibility  might  be  referable  to  the  membrane  possessing 
irreciprocal  permeability.  It  would  be  natural  to  find  in  the 
arc,  each  time  it  passed  through  gray  matter,  the  additive  intro- 
duction of  features  of  reaction  such  as  characterize  a  neurone- 
threshold  (Goldscheider).^^'^  The  conception  of  the  nervous 
impulse  as  a  physical  process  (du  Bois  Reymond)  rather  than 
a  chemical,  gains  rather  than  loses  plausibility  from  physical 
chemistry.  The  injury-current  of  nerve  seems  comparable  in 
mode  of  production  (J.  S.  Macdonald)  ^^  with  the  current  of  a 
"  concentration  cell,"  a  mode  of  energy  akin  to  the  expansion 
of  a  gas  and  physical,  rather  than  chemical,  *  volume-energy.* 
Against  the  Hkelihood  of  nervous  conduction  being  pre-emi- 
nently a  chemical  rather  than  a  physical  process  must  be  reck- 
oned, as  Macdonald  well  urges,  its  speed  of  propagation,  its 
brevity  of  time-relations,  its  freedom  from  perceptible  tempera- 
ture change,  its  facile  excitation  by  mechanical  means,  its 
facilitation  by  cold,  etc.  If  it  is  a  physical  process  the  inter- 
calation of  a  transverse  surface  of  separation  or  membrane  into 
the  conductor  must  modify  the  conduction,  and  it  would  do  so 
with  results  just  such  as  we  find  differentiating  reflex-arc  con- 
duction from  nerve-trunk  conduction. 


i8  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

As  to  the  existence  or  the  non-existence  of  a  surface  of 
separation  or  membrane  between  neurone  and  neurone,  that  is 
a  structural  question  on  which  histology  might  be  competent  to 
give  valuable  information.  In  certain  cases,  especially  in  In- 
vertebrata,  observation  (Apathy,  Bethe,  etc.)  indicates  that  many 
nerve-cells  are  actually  continuous  one  with  another.  It  is  note- 
worthy that  in  several  of  these  cases  the  irreversibility  of  direc- 
tion of  conduction  which  is  characteristic  of  spinal  reflex-arcs  is 
not  demonstrable ;  thus  the  nerve-net  in  some  cases,  e.g.  Medusa, 
exhibits  reversible  conduction  (Romanes,  Nagel,  Bethe,  and 
others).  But  in  the  neurone-chains  of  the  gray-centred  system 
of  vertebrates  histology  on  the  whole  furnishes  evidence  that  a 
surface  of  separation  does  exist  between  neurone  and  neurone. 
And  the  evidence  of  Wallerian  secondary  degeneration  is  clear 
in  showing  that  that  process  observes  strictly  a  boundary 
between  neurone  and  neurone  and  does  not  transgress  it.  It 
seems  therefore  Hkely  that  the  nexus  between  neurone  and  neu- 
rone in  the  reflex-arc,  at  least  in  the  spinal  arc  of  the  vertebrate, 
involves  a  surface  of  separation  between  neurone  and  neurone ; 
and  this  as  a  transverse  membrane  across  the  conductor  must 
be  an  important  element  in  intercellular  conduction.  The 
characters  distinguishing  reflex-arc  conduction  from  nerve- 
trunk  conduction  may  therefore  be  largely  due  to  intercellular 
barriers,  delicate  transverse  membranes,  in  the  former. 

In  view,  therefore,  of  the  probable  importance  physiologically 
of  this  mode  of  nexus  between  neurone  and  neurone  it  is  con- 
venient to  have  a  term  for  it.  The  term  introduced  has  been 
synapse}'^ 

The  differences  between  nerve-trunk  conduction  and  reflex- 
arc  conduction  are  so  great  as  to  require  for  their  exhibition  no 
very  minute  determination  of  the  characters  of  either ;  but  we 
may  with  advantage  follow  these  differences  somewhat  further. 
In  doing  so  we  may  take  the  reflexes  of  the  hind  limb  of  the 
spinal  dog  as  a  field  of  exemplification. 

Reflex  latency.  A  dissimilarity  between  nerve-trunk  con- 
duction and  reflex-arc  conduction  which  has  often  been  stressed 
is  the  slowness  of  the  latter  as  measured  by  the  latent  interval 


I]         LONG   LATENCY   OF  WEAK   REFLEXES  19 

between  application  of  stimulus  and  appearance  of  end-effect 
In  nerve-trunks  the  interval  between  the  moment  of  stimulation 
and  the  appearance  of  response  (electrical)  at  any  distant  point 
is  strictly  proportional  to  the  distance  of  that  point  from  the  seat 
of  stimulation.  There  is  in  the  nerve-trunk  no  measurable  delay 
or  latent  interval  for  the  response  at  the  seat  of  excitation. 
The  latent  time  for  nerve-trunk  response  is  therefore  entirely  a 
propagation  time.  The  speed  of  propagation  in  frog's  nerve 
at  15°  C.  is  about  3  cm.  per  sigma  ((t  =  .ooi  second).  We 
may  compare  with  this  the  latent  period  of  the  flexion-reflex 
of  the  "  spinal "  dog's  hind  leg.  The  movement  of  this  reflex 
is  a  flexion  at  knee,  hip,  and  ankle.  It  is  easily  and  regularly 
evoked  by  nocuous  or  electrical  stimuli  applied  to  the  skin  of 
the  limb  or  to  any  afferent  nerve  of  the  limb.  For  measure- 
ments of  the  reflex  latency  I  have  stimulated  with  break  or 
make  shocks  of  regular  but  varied  frequency.  Assuming  that 
in  warm-blooded  nerves  the  conduction  is  the  same  (Helmholtz 
found  it  faster)  as  in  the  frog,  and  that  the  length  of  the  reflex- 
arc  of  the  dog's  knee  is  two  thirds  of  a  metre,  and  assuming 
that  we  may  add  5  a-  for  mechanical  latency  of  the  flexor  con- 
traction of  the  limb,  we  should  have  about  27  <t  as  the  latent 
time  for  the  flexion-reflex,  supposing  its  conduction  proceeded 
as  does  nerve-trunk  conduction.  But,  as  a  fact,  a  period  double 
that  is  common  enough  for  this  reflex  under  ordinary  moderate 
intensities  of  stimulation. 

But  with  intenser  stimuli  the  latent  period  of  this  reflex  is 
much  less.  A  period  of  300-  from  commencement  of  stimu- 
lus to  commencement  of  mechanical  response  is  not  then  un- 
common. I  have  met,  at  shortest,  with  22  a.  There  is  here 
little  difference  between  speed  of  reflex  conduction  and  speed 
of  nerve-trunk  conduction.  Similarly  Francois  Franck"^  has 
recorded  latent  periods  for  reflex  action  differing  little  from 
those  of  simple  nerve-trunk  conduction.  Thus,  17  a  were  ob- 
tained for  a  reflex  contraction  of  the  crossed  gastrocnemius 
evoked  by  stimulation  of  the  afferent  root  of  the  first  lumbar 
nerve.  These  short  latencies  Franck  obtained  with  strong 
stimuli. 

It  would  seem,  therefore,  that  the  more  intense  the  stimulation 


20 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  2.  —  A,  B.  Scratch-reflex.  The  tracings  show  the  usual  lengthening  of  latency  on 
reducing  the  intensity  of  the  stimulation.  The  two  tracings  are  in  reproduction  unequally 
reduced,  but  the  frequency  of  repetition  of  the  double-induction  shocks  used  as  stimuli 
was  the  same  in  both  shocks  of  weaker  intensity  in  B  than  in  A.  The  reflex  movement 
began  after  delivery  of  three  stimuli  in  A,  after  delivery  of  nine  in  B.  The  greater 
intensity  of  the  stimuli  in  A  is  also  evidenced  by  the  greater  amplitude  of  the  movement 
and  by  the  longer  "  after-discharge."     Time  marked  in  fifths  of  seconds  below. 


I]         LONG   LATENCY  OF  WEAK   REFLEXES         21 

the  more  the  conduction  along  the  reflex-arc  comes  to  resemble 
in  speed  the  conduction  along  simple  nerve-trunks. 

It  is  with  mild  stimuli  that  the  difference  in  speed  between 
reflex  conduction  and  nerve-trunk  conduction  becomes  most 
obvious.  The  latent  period  for  the  flexion-reflex,  then,  lies 
usually  between  60  a  and  1200-.  I  have  met  with  it  as  long  as 
200  0-.  There  is  no  good  evidence  that  the  speed  of  propaga- 
tion in  nerve-trunk  conduction  is  in  response  to  weak  stimuli 
appreciably  slower  than  to  strong.  This  slackening  of  propa- 
gation speed  under  weak  stimuli  (Fig.  2)  is,  I  would  urge,  a  more 
significant  difference  between  reflex-conduction  and  nerve-trunk 
conduction  than  is  the  mere  greater  slowness  of  the  former  than 
of  the  latter.  Another  difference  between  the  two  in  regard  to 
conduction-speed  is  that  in  the  various  cerebrospinal  nerve- 
trunks  of  the  same  animal  species  the  conduction-speed  ap- 
pears to  be  practically  the  same.  But  reflex  conduction-speed 
as  measured  by  the  latent  period  differs  greatly  in  the  various 
type-reflexes  of  even  one  and  the  same  limb.  The  latent  time 
of  the  scratch-reflex  is,  on  the  average,  much  longer  than  that 
of  the  flexion-reflex  or  extensor-thrust,  although  the  spatial  dis- 
tance of  the  nerve-fibre  conduction  is  not  greater.  The  latency 
of  the  former  usually  in  my  experience  lies  between  140  a-  for 
intenser  stimulation  and  500  <t  for  weaker,  and  I  have  seen  it  ex- 
tend to  24400-  and  even  to  3540  o-.  So  that  although  a  weakly 
provoked  flexion-reflex  may  have  a  lengthier  latency  than  a 
strongly  provoked  scratch-reflex,  the  latency  of  the  scratch- 
reflex  is  nevertheless  on  the  average  very  characteristically 
longer  than  that  of  the  flexion-reflex.  Now  there  is  no  evidence 
that  this  is  referable  to  a  difference  in  the  conduction  rate  along 
the  nerve-trunks  of  the  two  reflexes ;  indeed,  the  efferent  nerve- 
trunks  for  the  two  reflexes  are  the  same. 

The  speed  of  travel  of  nervous  impulses  along  nerve-trunks 
is  fairly  known.  On  the  not  improbable  assumption  that  their 
velocity  along  the  myelinate  fibres  of  the  white  tracts  of  the 
central  nervous  system  is  about  the  same  as  along  the  myelinate 
fibres  of  nerve-trunks,  the  latent  period  of  reflex-actions  of 
moderate  intensity  is  obviously  greater  than  can  be  accounted 


22  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

for  by  travel  along  such  conductors  of  the  same  length  as  the 
reflex-arc  itself.  The  delay  in  speed  occurs  whenever  the 
impulses  pass  through  gray  matter.  This  has  been  especially 
clearly  shown  by  Exner."^  The  delay  in  the  gray  matter  may 
conceivably  be  due  to  slower  conduction  in  the  minute,  branched, 
and  more  diffuse  conducting  elements  —  perikarya,  dendrites, 
arborizations,  etc.  —  found  there;  or  it  may  be  referable  to  a 
fresh  kind  of  transmission  coming  in  there,  a  process  of  trans- 
mission different  in  nature  to  conduction  along  nerve-fibres. 
The  neurone  itself  is  visibly  a  continuum  from  end  to  end,  but 
continuity,  as  said  above,  fails  to  be  demonstrable  where  neurone 
meets  neurone  —  at  the  synapse.  There  a  different  kind  of  trans- 
mission may  occur.  The  delay  in  the  gray  matter  may  be  refer- 
able, therefore,  to  the  transmission  at  the  synapse. 

And  if  the  delay  occur  at  the  synapse,  the  possibility  sug- 
gests itself  that  the  time  consumed  in  the  latent  period  may 
be  spent  mainly  in  establishing  active  connection  along  the 
nervous-arc,  which  connection  once  established,  the  conduction 
in  the  arc  then  proceeds  perhaps  as  speedily  as  does  conduction 
in  a  simple  nerve-trunk.  The  latent  time  would  then  be  com- 
parable with  time  spent  in  closing  a  key  to  complete  an  electric 
circuit  or  in  setting  points  at  a  railway  junction.  The  key  once 
closed,  the  points  once  set,  the  transmission  is  as  expeditious 
there  as  elsewhere.  Measurements  of  reflex  times  deal  custom- 
arily, so  far  as  I  am  aware,  with  the  latent  time  of  reflexes  initiated 
in  arcs  not  in  action  at  the  moment  when  the  exciting  stimulus  is 
applied  to  their  afferent  end.  How  the  latent  time  is  spent  can 
receive  some  light  from  observation  on  the  latent  time  of  an 
increase  of  action  in  an  arc  already  active  in  the  same  direction 
as  the  incremental  action. 

To  examine  this  the  flexion-reflex  was  excited  by  a  sub- 
maximal  stimulus,  and  after  its  appearance  the  intensity  of  the 
exciting  stimulus  was  abruptly  increased  by  short-circuiting  a 
definite  resistance  from  the  primary  circuit.  The  stimulus  was 
a  series  of  break  shocks  of  regular  interval  given  by  a  key 
rotating  at  constant  speed  in  the  primary  circuit  of  the  induc- 
torium.     An   electromagnet  marked   the   interruptions  of  the 


I]    LATENT  TIME   OF  INCREMENTAL  REFLEX      23 


Figure  3.  —  Flexion-reflex.  Spinal  dog.  Latent  time  of  incremental  reflex  compared 
with  that  of  initial  reflex.  Unipolar  faradization  by  break  shocks.  Kathode  at  needle- 
point in  plantar  skin  of  outermost  digit.  A  weak  initial  stimulus  is  delivered  and  main« 
tained,  and  then  when  the  resulting  reflex  movement  has  become  steady  the  stimulus  is 
increased  in  intensity  by  short-circuiting  5  ohms  from  the  primary  circuit.  The  rate  and 
intensity  of  the  break  shocks  is  marked  above  by  a  recording  electromagnet ;  the  armature 
is  arranged  to  have  an  ampler  excursion  when  the  current  is  increased  at  the  point  marked 
B.  The  latent  time  of  the  incremental  reflex  is  seen  to  the  right  hand,  and  is  distinctly 
shorter  than  that  of  the  initial  reflex.  Time  below  is  written  in  iJ^  sec.  and  in  seconds. 
Abscissae  on  the  myograph  line  show  the  moment  of  first  delivery  of  the  initial  (A)  and 
of  the  incremental  ( B)  stimuli. 


24  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

primary  current;  the  electromagnet  was  arranged  to  show  by 
more  ample  excursion  of  its  armature  the  point  of  time  from 
which  onward  the  primary  current  was  increased.  The  shocks 
were  applied  by  a  needle-point  (kathode)  to  the  skin  of  a  digit : 
the  other  electrode,  large  and  diffuse,  was  wrapped  round  a  fore- 
foot, /.  e.,  head  ward  of  the  spinal  transection.  In  these  experi- 
ments the  earlier  reflex  elicited  may  be  termed  the  initial  reflex. 
Its  sudden  increase  on  sudden  intensification  of  the  stimulus 
may  be  termed  the  incremental  reflex.  The  latent  times  of  the 
initial  reflex  and  incremental  reflex,  when  compared,  showed 
almost  always  that  the  latency  of  the  latter  was  rather  the 
shorter.  But  the  difference  often  was  not  great  (Figs.  3  and  4). 
The  average  for  30  of  the  initial  reflexes  was  48  tr,  and  for  the 
30  corresponding  incremental  reflexes  was  38  c  This  difference 
seems  too  small  to  support  the  supposition  that  the  latent  time 
of  the  initial  reflex  is  chiefly  consumed  by  "  setting  "  the  synapse, 
which,  once  set,  conducts  in  much  the  same  way  as  regards  speed 
of  transmission  as  does  the  rest  of  the  arc.  It  might  be  that  the 
incremental  reaction  involved  the  "  setting  "  of  other  additional 
synapses.  But  such  an  explanation  demands  that  none  of  the 
augmentation  occur  through  the  synapses  already  in  action,  for 
the  latent  time  is  measured  to  the  first  beginning  of  the  steplike 
incremental  ascent  of  the  curve. 

In  the  incremental  stage  of  the  reaction  the  reflex  is  usually 
a  relatively  intense  one.  Now  the  length  of  reflex  latency  is 
caeteris  paribus  inversely  as  the  intensity  of  the  reflex.  If  the 
reflex  as  produced  in  two  stages  be  compared  with  the  reflex 
produced  by  delivery  of  the  stimulus  in  its  full  strength  at  the 
outset,  the  latent  time  of  this  latter  is  found  shorter  than  that  of 
either  the  initial  or  incremental  reaction  of  the  other  reflex 
(Fig.  4).  The  latent  time  under  the  same  external  stimulus  is 
thus  less  in  circumstances  that  ex  hypothesi  involve  building  a 
bridge  and  then  sending  impukes  across  it,  than  when  the  bridge 
already  having  been  built  the  impulses  have  merely  to  pass. 
This  argues  against  an  amoeboid  movement  of  the  protoplasm 
of  the  cell  being  the  step  which  determines  its  conductive  com- 
munication with  the  next  (Demoor,  Cajal,  Renaut,  Monti,  Duval, 


I]     LATENT   TIME   OF    INCREMENTAL   REFLEX      25 


Figure  4.  —  Same  as  preceding  (Fig.  3),  but  with  somewhat  stronger  initial  stimulus  and 
reflex.  At  the  extreme  right  hand  of  the  figure  is  shown  the  beginning  of  the  "  total "  re» 
flex,  that  is,  the  reflex  when  the  intensity  of  stimulus  used  incrementally  in  the  left-hand 
observation  is  thrown  in  at  outset.  There  is  little  difference  between  the  latent  times  of 
the  "initial"  and  "incremental"  reflexes;  the  latent  time  of  the  "total"  reflex  is 
shorter  than  that  of  either  the  initial  or  incremental  reflex.  Electromagnet  above  records 
break  shocks  as  before ;  at  A  the  initial^  at  B  the  incremental^  at  y  the  total  stimulus. 
The  time  below  is  given  in  ^j^  sec.  and  in  seconds. 

Lugaro).  It  also  seems  conclusive  against  any  major  portion  of  the 
latent  period  being  consumed  at  the  synapse  in  a  process  which 
sets  the  synapse  ready  to  conduct,  —  a  process  of  preparation 
for  transmission  as  distinguished  from  a  process  of  transmission. 
It  argues  that  the  delay  is  inherent  in  the  process  of  transmis- 
sion itself,  and  that  therefore  the  actual  nervous  transmission 
at  these  points  has,  when  the  stimuli  are  weak,  a  different  order 
of  speed  to  that  in  nerve-fibres.  The  shorter  latent  time  of  the 
reflex  induced  by  the  stimulus  delivered  in  full  at  the  outset  is  in 


26  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

harmony  with  the  reflex,  being  under  that  mode  of  excitation 
rather  more  intense  {e,  g.^  of  greater  ampHtude)  than  when 
excited  as  an  increment  to  a  foregoing  reflex  of  less  strength. 
The  shorter  latent  times  given  by  intenser  stimuli  seem  readily 
explicable  by  the  minimal  quantity  of  transmitted  influence  nec- 
essary to  give  detectible  effect,  being  necessarily  earlier  reached 
with  copious  transmission  than  with  weaker  transmission. 

The  observations  indicate,  therefore,  that  the  latent  time 
belongs  to  some  process  which  is  the  same  in  nature,  both  in 
initiating  a  reflex  from  a  resting  arc  and  in  increasing  a  reflex 
through  an  arc  already  in  submaximal  activity,  —  and  probably 
therefore  in  maintaining  a  reflex  in  unaltered  continuance  in  an 
arc.  It  argues  that  any  "  setting  "  process  in  the  nerve-centre, 
if  it  occur  at  all,  is  negligible  in  regard  to  the  time  it  consumes- 
It  suggests  that  even  while  at  rest  the  reflex  apparatus  is  just 
as  prepared  for  immediately  transmitting  impulses  as  when 
actually  engaged  in  reflex  activity  in  the  very  direction  the  new 
impulses  would  require.  It  therefore  suggests  the  greater  need 
for  active  inhibition  in  the  co-ordination  of  activity  of  arcs  which 
have  a  final  path  in  common  and  yet  use  that  path  to  different 
effects.  If  resting  paths  all  lie  open  for  conduction,  prevention 
of  confusion  must  depend  not  on  the  path  excited  being  the 
only  one  open  for  conduction,  but  on  its  excitation  being  accom- 
panied by  inhibition  of  others  that,  did  they  enter  into  action, 
would  detrimentally  confuse  the  issue  of  events. 

Reflex  after-discharge.  Another  characteristic  difference  be- 
tween conduction  in  nerve-trunks  and  in  reflex-arcs  is  the  less 
close  correspondence  in  the  latter  between  moment  of  cessation 
of  stimulus  and  moment  of  cessation  of  end-effect.  The  reflex- 
arc  shows  marked  "  after-discharge  ** ;  the  nerve-trunk  does  not. 
Tetanic  contraction  of  the  knee-flexor  muscles  of  the  dog  in- 
duced by  brief  faradization  of  the  motor-nerve  usually  ceases 
within  1500-  of  the  cessation  of  the  stimulation  of  the  nerve, 
if  crude  condition  of  fatigue,  etc.,  be  avoided.  The  contraction 
of  those  same  muscles,  when  induced  reflexly  by  a  similar  brief 
stimulation,  often  persists  for  ^Q00(t  after  cessation  of  the  stimu- 
lus (Fig.  5). 


I] 


AFTER-DISCHARGE 


27 


Figure  5.  —  Flexion-reflex.     Effect  of  increase  of  intensity  of  stimulation.     Stimulus  con- 
sisted of  45  break  shocks  delivered  at  frequency  of  42  per  second  for  each  reflex. 


Iniensity  of  stimulus. 

I. 

69 

11. 

no 

III. 

190 

IV. 

300 

Time  in  seconds  below.     Cloni 

28  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

Measure  0/ reflex.  Measure  0/  after-discharge. 

110  50 

273  161 

782  626 

II96  IO16 

Clonus  is  seen  in  the  after-discharge.  The  measure  of  intensity 
of  stimulus  is  given  from  the  units  of  the  Kronecker  coil;  the  measure  of  the  reflex  is 
from  the  area  included  between  myograph  curve  and  base  line.    Reflex  IV  not  in  the  figure. 

We  must  subtract  from  the  period  of  after-discharge  a  period 
equal  to  the  latent  time.  But  usually  the  latent  time  is  quite 
insignificant  in  length  as  compared  with  the  after-discharge. 
The  after-discharge  in  flexion-reflex  VI  (Fig.  6)  was  more  than 
five  hundred  times  longer  than  the  latent  time. 

The  after-discharge  increases  with  increase  of  intensity  of  the 
stimulus,  not  only  absolutely,  but  relatively  to  the  whole  reflex  — 
when  the  stimulation  is  not  long  lasting.  Taking  the  flexion- 
reflex  for  example,  the  increase  of  after-discharge  with  increas- 
ing intensity  of  stimulation  is  more  marked  than  the  increase  of 
contraction-height  (Fig.  7).  With  stimulation  lasting  not  more 
than  1000  <T  the  maximum  amplitude  of  the  reflex  arrives,  as  the 
intensity  of  the  stimulus  is  increased,  later  and  later,  so  that  with 
weaker  stimuli  it  falls  within  the  excitation  period,  but  with 
stronger  stimuli  it  is  reached  only  after  the  application  of  the 
stimulus  has  ceased.  Very  marked  in  the  after-discharge  of 
this,  the  flexion-reflex,  is  a  clonus  (Figs.  5,  6)  with  a  rate  in  my 
records  varying  between  7.5  and  12  per  second.  Undulations 
of  similar  kinds  are  clear  in  the  reflex  movement  even  from 
the  outset  in  weak  intensities  of  the  reflex. 

Figure  6.  —  Flexion-reflex.  Stimulus  for  each  reflex  was  72  break  shocks  at  the  rate  of  40 
per  second.  This  stimulus  is  registered  above  by  electromagnet  in  the  primary.  Ab- 
scissae on  the  myograph  curve  show  its  delivery  and  cessation  on  that  curve.  The  curve 
marked  V  is  the  actual  record  of  its  observation  ;  the  other  curves  are  tracings  of  the 
records  obtained  in  the  consecutive  series  of  which  curve  V  was  the  fifth  member;  the 
record  is  thus  condensed  and  contrasted. 

Intensity  0/ stimulus.  Measure  0/ reflex.  Measure  0/  after-dischargt. 

I.  350  94  51 

II.  475  200  119 

III.  690  666  509 

IV.  1 100  913  692 
V.  '     1900             1277  964 

VI.     3000  1765  1432 

VII.      350  62  34 

The  clonus  of  the  after-discharge  is  well  seen.     Time  below  in  seconds. 


I] 


AFTER-DISCHARGE 


29 


< 

1 
I 

c 

1        c 
c 

C^2^       -«^C                         ) 

1 

^ 

^ 

^•^" 

1 

,       1 

^|r\_,,^      Jl 

Figure  6. — Flexion-reflex. 


30  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

Under  short-lasting  application  of  rather  weak  serial  stimuli 
at  slow  frequency  of  repetition  (break  shocks  at  20  per  second), 
increase  of  the  number  of  stimuli,  without  alteration  of  their  in- 
tensity or  rate,  in  other  words  mere  prolongation  of  the  stimula- 
tion, increases  the  after-discharge  (Fig.  8).  The  reflex  induced 
by  nine  stimuli  has  an  after-discharge  three  times  as  great  as  the 
reflex  from  three  similar  stimuli.  The  maximum  amplitude  in 
this  case  may  remain  practically  the  same,  the  later  stimuli 
simply  prolonging  the  maximum  without  increasing  it.  They 
prolong  it  for  a  far  greater  time  than  their  own  delivery  pro- 
longs the  stimulation  time. 

In  the  scratch-reflex,  likewise,  intensity  increases  the  after- 
discharge  (Fig.  9).  Its  after-discharge  is  rhythmic,  a  clonus 
like  the  rest  of  the  reflex,  with  the  slight  lengthening  of  duration 
and  sequence  of  the  terminal  beats  that  is  characteristic  of  this 
reflex.  The  after-discharge  from  the  scratch-reflex  is  not 
usually  so  prolonged  as  that  from  a  flexion-reflex  produced  by 
a  stimulus  of  like  length  and  intensity  (Fig.  10).  Six  to  nine 
beats  usually  complete  the  after-discharge. 

In  the  spinal  dog  there  is  a  reflex  of  the  hind  limb  in  which 
a  movement  of  extension  at  knee,  ankle,  and  hip  is  caused 
by  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  contralateral  hind  limb  —  the 
"crossed  extension-reflex."  When  this  reflex  is  provoked  with 
more  than  a  certain  intensity  its  after-discharge  becomes  a 
feature  of  extraordinary  prominence,  both  as  regards  degree  of 
contraction  and  duration.  This  after-discharge  may  then  be 
more  intense  than  any  other  part  of  the  reflex,  and  may  persist, 
gradually  decHning  for  10  or  15  seconds  (Fig.  27).  Wundt®^ 
and  Biedermann  ^^  have  noted  that  in  the  cooled  frog  the  dura- 
tion of  the  reflex  after-discharge  is  prolonged. 

FiGURB  7.  —  Flexion-reflex.  Elicited  by  lo  break  shocks  at  rate  of  20  per  second,  «.#., 
stimulation  Isisting  .5".  Intensity  of  stimuli  increased  by  bringing  secondary  coil  toward 
primary. 

Stimulus.  Whole  refitx.  After-discharge, 

Top  reflex  30  12  9 

Next    "  45  52  43 

Next    "  65  130  118 

Next    "  85  176  158 

Bottom  reflex         100  258  236 

The  intensity  of  stimulus  is  given  in  units  of  the  Kronecker  inductorium.    The  amount 
of  reflex  is  measured  by  the  area  between  the  myograph  curve  and  the  base  line. 


I] 


AFTER-DISCHARGE 


31 


Figure  7.—  Flexion-reflex. 


32 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  8.  —  Flexion-reflex;  "  spinal  "  dog.     Effect  of  prolongation  of  the  exciting  stimuu-     i 
tion.     The  circular  key  rotating  at  constant  speed  interrupted  the  primary  circuit  of  an      \ 


V 


AFTER-DISCHARGE 


33 


inductorium  the  break  shocks  of  which  were  delivered  through  a  needle  electrode  (kath- 
ode)  to  the  plantar  skin  of  the  outermost  digit.  An  adjustible  spring  rheotome  allowed 
the  desired  number  of  interruptions  in  the  primary,  and  therefore  the  desired  number  of 
break  shocks  in  the  exciting  circuit  when  that  was  unshortcircuited.  Three  of  the  suc- 
cessive stimuli  elicited  the  uppermost  reflex,  four  the  next,  five  the  next,  six  the  next, 
and  nine  the  lowest.  The  "  af ter-discharge  "  is  seen  to  be  increased  by  mere  prolonga- 
tion of  the  stimulus  within  these  limits.  The  frequency  of  the  stimuli  remained  in  all 
cases  20  per  second,  and  their  intensity  was  the  same  for  all  the  reflexes. 


Duration  o/sHmuius. 

Measure  o/i 

reflex. 

Measure 

0/  a/ter-disckarg*. 

I50,r 

20 

19 

200  O- 

37 

34 

250* 

53 

47 

300  «r 

58 

49 

450<r 

70 

58 

Time  in  seconds  above  lowest  record. 


Figure  9.  —  Effect  of  intensity  of  stimulus  on  scratch-reflex.  Stimulus  is  9  break  shocks  at 
rate  of  25  per  second  delivered  to  a  point  in  the  scapular  skin  by  unipolar  faradization, 
the  stigmatic  electrode  being  the  kathode.  A,  the  stimulus  is  very  weak :  one  small  beat 
of  characteristic  slowness  is  evoked  after  a  long  latent  period ;  B,  increase  in  intensity  of 
shocks  with  resulting  shorter  latent  time  and  a  reflex  movement  of  two  feeble  beats ; 
C,  further  increase  of  intensity  of  stimulus :  the  latent  time  is  shorter,  and  a  reflex  of  ten 
fairly  quick  and  ample  beats  ensues.  The  stimulus  lasted  less  than  a  half  second ;  the 
reflex  is  not  completed  for  more  than  two  seconds  after  cessation  of  the  stimulus. 

There  is  no  feature  of  the  conduction  of  a  reflex-arc  which 
distinguishes  its  mechanism  more  universally  from  that  of  a 
mere  nerve-fibre  tract  or  trunk  than  lengthy  after-discharge. 
Richet^^  has  paradoxically  applied  to  this  feature  the  old 
adage  modified :    '*  Sublata  causa,  non  tollitur  efifectus."     The 

3 


34 


THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


after-discharge  can,  however,  be  cut  short  sharply  by  "  inhibi- 
tion" \  it  seems  also  to  remanifest  itself  sometimes  after  a 
passing  interruption  by  inhibition. 

The  long  latency  and  the  marked  after-discharge  of  reflex- 
conduction  easily  explain  a  phenomenon  often  met  when  study- 
ing reflexes  provoked  by  stimuli  that  are  brief,  especially  if  they 


Figure  io.  — Scratch-reflex  and  flexion-reflex  provoked  by  similar  stimulation  in  the  same 
animal  in  quick  succession.  Stimulation  was  unipolar  faradization  with  45  break  shocks ; 
the  kathode  was  stigmatic  and  applied,  to  the  shoulder  skin  for  scratch-reflex,  to  the 
fourth  toe  for  flexion-reflex;  diffuse  electrode  on  forefoot.  Frequency  of  shocks,  18 
per  second.  The  after-discharge  of  the  scratch-reflex  lasted  barely  one  second ;  that  of 
the  flexion,  nearly  eight  seconds.  Time  is  marked  in  seconds  below  each  record  ;  above, 
an  electromagnet  in  the  primary  circuit  records  the  interruptions  giving  the  shocks. 


be  also  weak.  The  stimulus,  thaugh  it  may  last  for  a  good 
many  sigmata,  is  over  and  past  ere  the  reflex-response  appears 
(Fig-  9)'  That  response,  when  it  appears,  may  nevertheless 
endure  for  looo  a  or  more.  There  is  nothing  closely  compar- 
able with  this  in  the  conduction  of  nerve-trunks. 

The  after-discharge  of  a  reflex  may  be  considered  analogous 
to  2i  positive  after-image  left  by  a  visual  stimulus.    The  analogy 


I]  AFTER-DISCHARGE  35 

is  suggestive  in  connection  with  others  to  be  drawn  between 
spinal  and  visual  phenomena. 

Conduction  along  reflex-arcs  presents  in  contrast  to  that 
along  nerve-trunks  characters  that  may  be  figuratively  described 
as  indicating  inertia  and  momentum.  It  is  as  though  in  the 
case  of  a  weight  to  be  pulled  from  a  position  of  rest  the  tractive 
force  were  applied  through  a  rigid  rod  in  nerve-trunk  conduc- 
tion, but  through  a  relatively  yielding  elastic  band  in  reflex-arc 
conduction.  But  there  are  other  differences  between  the  two 
forms  of  conduction  which  this  simple  simile  does  not  figure. 
We  have  to  enter  on  such  at  our  next  meeting. 


! 


36  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 


LECTURE    II 

CO-ORDINATION   IN   THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  {continued) 

Argument:  Reflex-arcs  show  high  capacity  for  summing  excitations. 
Irreversibility  of  direction  of  conduction  in  reflex-arcs.  Reversibility 
of  direction  of  conduction  in  certain  nerve-nets,  e.  g.  that  of  Medusa. 
Independence  between  the  rhythm  of  the  reflex-discharge  and  the 
rhythm  of  the  external  stimulus  exciting  it.  Refractory  phase  in  re- 
flexes ;  in  the  eyelid-reflex ;  in  the  scratch -reflex.  The  neuronic 
construction  of  the  reflex-arc  of  the  scratch-reflex.  Long  descend- 
ing propriO'Spinal  tracts  revealed  by  the  method  of  "successive 
degeneration."  The  "final  common  path"  and  the  "afferent  arc." 
Intraspinal  seat  of  the  refractory  phase  of  the  scratch-reflex.  The 
value  of  refractory  phase  in  the  co-ordination  of  the  swimming  of 
Medusa.  Its  value  in  the  co-ordination  of  the  scratch-reflex.  Signifi- 
cance of  the  intraspinal  situation  of  the  refractory  phase  of  the  scratch- 
reflex.     Other  instances  of  "  central "  refractory  phase. 

Summation.  Summation  of  subliminal  stimuli  so  that  by  repe- 
tition they  become  effective  is  practically  unknown  in  nerve- 
trunk  conduction.  But  it  is  a  marked  feature  of  reflex-arc 
conduction  (vSetschenovir,^^  Stirling^'').  Nor  is  it  attributable  to 
the  muscles  whose  contraction  may  serve  as  index  of  the  reflex- 
response,  since  summation  of  this  extent  is  not  known  for 
vertebrate  skeletal  muscle,  though  found  by  Richet^  in  the 
claw-muscle  of  the  crayfish. 

We  find  striking  instances  of  the  summation  of  subliminal 
stimuli  given  by  the  scratch-reflex.  The  difficulty  in  excit- 
ing a  reflex  by  a  single-induction  shock  is  well  known.  A 
scratch-reflex  cannot  in  my  experience  be  elicited  by  a  single- 
induction  shock,  or  even  by  two  shocks,  unless  as  physiological 
stimuli  they  are  very  intense  and  delivered  less  than  600  a  apart. 
Although  the  strongest  single-induction  shock  is  therefore  by 
itself  a  subminimal  stimulus  for  this  reflex,  the  summating  power 
of  this  reflex   mechanism  is  great.     Very  feeble  shocks,  each 


su 


]  SUMMATION  37 


ucceeding  the  other  within  a  certain  time  —  summation  time  — 
sum  as  stimuli  and  provoke  a  reflex.  Thus  long  series  of  sub- 
minimal stimuli  ultimately  provoke  the  reflex.  I  have  records 
where  the  reflex  appeared  only  after  delivery  of  the  fortieth  suc- 
cessive double  shock,  the  shocks  having  followed  each  other  at  a 
frequency  of  1 1.3  per  second,  and  where  the  reflex  appeared  only 
after  delivery  of  the  forty-fourth  successive  make  shock,  the  shocks 
having  followed  at  18  per  second.  A  momentary  stimulus,  e.^.,a, 
break  shock  of  fair  physiological  strength  applied  by  a  stigmatic 
pole  (needle  point)  to  a  skin-spot  in  the  receptive  field  of  this 
reflex,  produces  in  the  nervous  arc  a  change  which  though,  as 
just  said,  unable  of  itself  alone  to  produce  the  reflex  movement, 
shows  its  facilitating  influence  {bahnung)  on  a  subsequent  stimu- 
lus applied  even  1400  <r  later.  The  duration  of  the  excitatory 
change  induced  by  a  momentary  stimulus  is  therefore  in  this 
mammalian  arc  (scratch-reflex)  almost  as  long  as  that  noted  in 
the  frog  by  Stirling,  namely,  1 500  (t. 

With  serial  stimuli  of  the  same  frequency  of  repetition  the 
latent  time  of  the  scratch-reflex  is  shorter  the  more  intense 
the  individual  stimuli.  Stirling  ^"^  conclusively  traced  length  of 
latency  to  dependence  on  spinal  summation  of  successive  excita- 
tions. In  accord  with  this  in  the  "scratch-reflex,"  when  the 
serial  stimuli  follow  slowly,  the  reflex  caeteris  paribus  is  pro- 
longed. A  single  brief  mechanical  stimulation  of  the  skin  (rub, 
prick,  or  pull  upon  a  hair)  usually  succeeds  in  exciting  a  scratch- 
reflex,  though  the  reflex  thus  evoked  is  short ;  but  there  is  noth- 
ing to  show  that  these  stimuli,  though  brief,  are  really  simple 
and  not  essentially  multiple.  A  striking  dissimilarity,  therefore, 
between  reflex-arc  conduction  and  nerve-trunk  conduction  is 
that  in  reflex-arc  conduction  considerable  resistance  is  offered 
to  the  passage  of  a  single  nerve-impulse,  but  the  resistance  is 
easily  forced  by  a  succession  of  impulses ;  in  other  words,  sub- 
liminal stimuli  are  summed. 

It  follows  almost  as  a  corollary  from  this  that  the  threshold 
excitability  of  a  reflex  mechanism  appears  much  more  variable 
than  that  of  a  nerve-trunk,  if  the  threshold  excitability  be  meas- 
ured in  terms  of  the  intensity  of  the  liminal  stimulus.    The  value 


38  THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

will  be  more  variable  in  the  case  of  the  reflex  mechanism,  be- 
cause there  the  duration  of  the  stimulus  is  a  factor  in  its  effi- 
ciency far  more  than  in  the  case  of  the  nerve-trunk.  In  the 
scratch-reflex  a  single  stimulus  which  is  far  below  threshold 
intensity  is  found,  on  its  fortieth  repetition  and  nearly  four 
seconds  after  its  first  appplication,  to  become  effective  and  pro- 
voke the  reflex. 

Irreversibility  of  direction  of  conduction.  Another  remarkable 
difference  between  reflex-arc  conduction  and  nerve-trunk  con- 
duction is  the  irreversibility  of  direction  of  the  former  and  the 
reversibility  of  the  latter.  Double  conduction,  as  it  has  been 
termed,  is  well-established  for  nerve-trunks  both  afferent  and 
efferent.  It  was  shown  by  du  Bois  Reymond  for  the  spinal 
nerve  roots,  for  peripheral  nerves  by  Kuhne's  gracilis  experi- 
ment, for  the  great  single  electric  fibre  of  Malapterurus  by 
Babuchin,^^  for  sympathetic  nerve-cords  by  Langley  and  Ander- 
son,^^  and  by  myself  ^^^  for  certain  fibres  of  the  white  tracts  of 
the  spinal  cord.  The  nerve-fibres  in  all  these  cases,  when  ex- 
cited anywhere  in  their  course,  conduct  nerve-impulses  in  all 
directions  from  the  point  stimulated ;  that  is,  in  their  case  both 
up  and  down,  the  only  two  directions  open  to  them.  Their 
substance  may  therefore  be  regarded  as  conductive  in  all  direc- 
tions along  their  extension. 

From  the  Bell-Magendie  law  of  the  spinal  nerve-roots  we 
know  that  reflex-arcs  conduct  only  in  one  direction.  The 
stimulation  of  the  central  end  of  a  motor-nerve  remains  without 
obvious  effect.  Bell  ^^  and  Magendie  ^^  and  their  followers  estab- 
lished that  excitation  of  the  spinal  end  of  the  severed  motor  root 
evokes  no  sign  of  reflex  action  or  sensation.  Evidently  the 
central  nexus  between  afferent  channel  and  efferent  is  of  a  kind 
that,  though  it  allows  conduction  from  afferent  to  efferent,  does 
not  allow  it  from  efferent  to  afferent.  The  path  is  patent  in  one 
direction  only.  This  is  the  special  case  which  forms  the  first 
foundation  of  the  law  that  conduction  in  the  neural  system  pro- 
ceeds in  one  direction  only,  the  "  law  of  forward  direction  (W. 
James,  1880).^^  When  the  property  of  double  conduction  in 
nerve-fibres  had  been  ascertained,  the  Bell-Magendie  law  of  the 


II]  IRREVERSIBLE   CONDUCTION  39 

spinal  roots  became  more  instructive.  Gad  ^^  (1884)  argued 
that  the  dendrites  of  the  motor  root-cell  are  capable  of  conduc- 
tion in  one  direction  only,  namely,  toward  and  not  away  from 
the  axone.  It  may,  however,  be  that  the  irreciprocity  of  the 
conduction  is  referable  to  the  synapse.  The  explanation  of  the 
valved  condition  of  the  reflex  circuit  may  lie  in  a  synaptic  mem- 
brane more  permeable  in  one  direction  than  in  the  other.  In 
other  words,  through  intraneuronic  conduction  is  reversible 
in  direction,  interneuronic  may  be  irreversible. 

Cell-chains  of  polarized  conduction  form  the  basis  of  the 
great  majority  of  all  the  nervous  reactions  of  the  cerebrospinal 
system  of  higher  animals.  It  appears,  however,  that  not  all 
pluricellular  nervous  circuits  exhibit  irreversible  direction  of 
conduction.  The  nerve-net  of  Medusa  is  a  pluricellular  con- 
ductor which  exhibits  reversibility  of  direction  of  conduction. 
In  Medusa  locomotion  is  effected  by  contraction  of  a  sheet  of 
muscle  in  the  swimming-bell.  When  the  swimming-bell,  which 
resembles  an  inverted  cup,  contracts,  its  capacity  is  lessened, 
and  some  of  the  water  embraced  by  it  is  expelled  through  the 
open  end,  the  animal  itself  being  propelled  in  the  reverse  direc- 
tion by  recoil.  The  mechanism  is  like  that  of  the  heart,  but 
the  heart  propels  its  contents,  the  swimming-bell  propels  itself 
against  its  contents.  The  contractions  of  both  recur  r>^thmi- 
cally,  though  Medusa,  unlike  the  heart,  has  periods  of  prolonged 
diastolic  inactivity.  At  such  a  period  an  appropriate  stimulus 
restarts  the  swimming-bell.  The  contractive  beat  begins  from 
the  point  stimulated  and  spreads  thence  over  the  whole  muscu- 
lar sheet.''*  It  spreads  rapidly  enough  for  the  contraction  not 
to  have  culminated  at  the  initial  point  before  it  has  set  in  at  the 
most  remote  part.  The  beat  is  thus  not  only  everywhere  in 
progress  at  the  same  time,  but  is  practically  in  the  same  phase  of 
progress  everywhere,  and  similarly  synchronously  passes  off."^^ 

The  arrangement  of  the  nervous  system  of  Medusa,  e.  g., 
Rhizostoma,  is,  according  to  Bethe,  of  the  following  kind 
(Fig.  II).  The  nerve-cell  has  on  one  hand  thread-like  arms 
that  extend  to  the  surface  of  the  subumbrella,  and  on  the  other 
hand  others  which  stretch  down  to  the  sheet  of  contractile  cells 


40 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  ii  (A.  Bethe  2W). —  Nerve-net  of  Rhizostoma.  A,  radial  section  through  a  mus- 
cular field  of  the  subumbrella ;  Ep,  epithelium ;  i«,  muscle-fibres  in  cross-section;  M.  K.^ 
their  nuclei ;  N.ply  nerve-plexus  with  fibres  running  into  the  epithelium  and  to  the  muscles; 
B,  nerve-plexus  with  scattered  cells,  from  a  horizontal  section.  Magnification  1200  in  A, 
200  in  B. 

on  the  under  side  of  the  bell.  Each  nerve-cell  has  also  long 
side  threads  which  join  similar  side  threads  from  other  nerve- 
cells.  By  virtue  of  these  lateral  connections  the  nerve-cells  form 
a  network  of  conductors  spreading  horizontally  through  the  bell 
in  a  layer  of  tissue  between  a  receptive  sheet  and  a  contractile 
sheet.  From  this  nerve-net,  throughout  its  extent,  there  pass 
nerve- threads  to  the  adjacent  muscle ;  it  also  receives  at  many 
points  of  its  extent  nerve-threads  from  specially  receptive  areas 
of  surface. 


II]     IRREVERSIBLE   CONDUCTION   AT   SYNAPSE    41 

The  circularly  arranged  sheet  of  muscle  does  not  form  a 
continuous  field  toward  the  centre  of  the  disc ;  there  are  wide 
radial  gaps  in  it.  Across  these  gaps  the  "conduction"  passes: 
the  microscope  reveals  no  muscular  tissue  in  these  gaps,  but  the 
nerve-net  can  be  seen  to  spread  across  them.^^  The  presence 
of  the  nerve-net  explains  the  conduction  across  them.  It  is 
therefore  argued  by  Bethe  that  the  spread  of  the  contraction 
over  the  muscular  sheet  in  Rhizostoma  does  not  imply  conduc- 
tion of  the  contraction  from  one  muscle-cell  to  another,  but  is 
the  result  of  the  spread  of  nervous  action  over  the  nerve-net 
work.  In  its  progress  along  the  nerve-net,  the  nervous  discharge, 
as  it  reaches  each  part  of  the  nerve-net,  spreads  down  the  nerve- 
threads,  descending  thence  to  the  underlying  muscle-sheet.  So 
long  as  the  nerve-cell  network  is  intact,  wherever  the  point 
stimulated,  the  ensuing  contraction  is  of  the  whole  bell,  that  is, 
the  nerve-impulses  started  at  one  point  of  the  receptive  surface, 
on  entering  the  nerve  network,  spread  over  it  in  all  directions. 
When  the  bell-shaped  disc  is  spirally  cut  into  a  long  band,  to 
whichever  end  of  the  band  the  stimulus  be  applied,  the  conduc- 
tion spreads  from  that  end  to  the  other  and  over  the  whole 
strip  (Romanes).'^^  The  nerve-net  therefore  conducts  nerve- 
impulses  in  both  directions  along  its  length.  Therefore  it  is  not 
a  polarized  conductor,  conductive  in  one  direction  only.  In  the 
chains  of  nerve-cells  of  higher  animals,  such  as  Arthropods  and 
Vertebrates,  although  the  conduction  is  reversible  in  each  nerve- 
cell,  —  at  least  along  that  piece  of  it  which  forms  a  nerve-fibre,  — 
the  pluricellular  chain  in  toto  constitutes  a  polarized  conductor, 
conductive  in  one  direction  only.  In  such  cell-chains  the  indi- 
vidual nerve-cells  are  characterized  morphologically  by  possess- 
ing two  kinds  of  cell-branches,  which  differ  one  from  another 
in  microscopic  form,  the  one  kind  dendrites,  the  other  axones. 
The  difference  in  appearance  between  dendrites  and  axones  is 
marked  enough  for  recognition  by  microscopical  inspection. 
Since  in  many  well-known  instances  the  dentrites  conduct  im- 
pulses away  from  their  free  ends,  while  the  axone  conducts 
towards  its  free  end,  it  is  possible  on  mere  microscopic  inspection 
of  nerve-cells  of  this  type  to  infer  by  analogy  the  normal  direction 


42  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

of  the  conduction  through  the  nerve-cell.  But  in  the  nerve-cells 
forming  the  nerve-network  of  Medusa  there  seems  no  such  dis- 
tinct differentiation  of  their  branches  into  two  types.  Their  cell- 
processes  are  not  distinguishable  into  dendrites  and  axones. 

Moreover,  microscopic  examination  of  the  nerve-net  of 
Medusa  reveals  another  difference  between  it  and  the  nerve-cell 
chains  of  higher  animals.  In  these  latter  the  neuro-fibrils  of  one 
nerve-cell  are  not  found  unbrokenly  continuous  with  those  of 
the  next  cell  along  the  nerve-chain.  Although  the  union  may 
be  close,  there  is  not  homogeneous  continuity.  The  one  nerve- 
cell  joins  another  by  synapsis.  But  in  the  nerve-net  of  Medusa 
the  neuro-fibrils  pass,  according  to  Bethe,  uninterruptedly  across 
from  one  cell  to  another.  Even  if  we  admit  the  neuro-fibrils  to 
be  in  a  measure  artifacts,  the  appearance  of  their  continuity 
from  one  cell  to  another  in  one  type,  and  of  their  discontinuity 
from  one  cell  to  another  in  the  other  type  remains  significant  of 
a  difference  between  the  conduction-process  from  cell  to  cell  in 
the  two  types.  The  nerve-net  of  Medusa  appears  an  unbroken 
retiform  continuum  from  end  to  end.  Each  nerve-cell  in  it 
joins  its  neighbours  much  as  at  a  node  in  the  myelinate  nerve- 
fibre  the  axis-cylinder  of  each  segment  joins  the  next.  Reversi- 
bility of  conduction  may  be  related  to  this  apparent  continuity 
of  structure,  and  irreversibility  to  want  of  it.  This  points  to  the 
latter's  being  referable  to  the  synapse ;  if  the  synaptic  membrane 
(Lect.  I.  p.  1 8)  be  permeable  only  in  one  direction  to  certain 
ions,  that  may  explain  the  irreversibility  of  conduction.  The 
polarised  conduction  of  nerve-arcs  would  be  related  to  the 
one-sided  permeability  of  the  intestinal  wall,  e.  g,  to  NaCl 
(O.  Cohnheim). 

Rhythm  of  response.  One  of  the  differences  between  nerve- 
trunk  conduction  and  reflex-arc  conduction  is  the  less  close 
correspondence  in  the  latter  between  rhythm  of  stimulus  and 
rhythm  of  end-effect.  The  number  of  separable  excitatory 
states  (impulses)  engendered  in  a  nerve-trunk  by  serially  re- 
peated stimuli  corresponds  closely  with. the  stimuli  in  number 
and  rhythm.  Whether  the  stimuli  follow  each  other  once  per 
second  or  five  hundred  times  per  second,  the  nervous  responses 


II]  RHYTHM    OF   END-EFFECT  43 

follow  the  rhythm  of  stimulation.  Using  contraction  of  skeletal 
muscle  as  index  of  the  response  the  correspondence  at  rhythms 
above  thirty  per  second  becomes  difficult  to  trace,  because  the 
mechanical  effects  tend  at  rates  beyond  that  to  fuse  indistin- 
guishably.  The  electrical  responses  of  the  muscle  can  with  ease 
be  observed  isolatedly  up  to  faster  rates :  their  rhythm  is  found 
to  agree  with  that  of  stimulation ;  thus,  at  eighty  per  second 
their  responses  are  eighty  per  second.  If  the  muscle  note  be 
accepted  as  an  indication  of  the  response  of  the  muscle,  its  pitch 
follows /^r/  passu  the  rate  of  stimulation  of  the  nerve  through 
a  still  greater  range. 

The  case  is  quite  different  with  reflex-arcs.  Schafer  ^^  noted 
undulations  of  a  frequency  of  ten  to  twelve  per  second  on  myo- 
grams of  spinal  reflexes  evoked  by  excitation  of  the  afferent 
nerve  by  faradic  currents  of  frequency  much  above  ten  to  twelve 
per  second.  In  such  a  case  we  may  assume  the  absence  in  the 
afferent  nerve  itself  of  any  refractory  period  long  enough  to  give 
a  ten  per  second  rhythm  to  the  response.  The  refractory  period 
in  nerve-trunk  conduction  seems  to  last  not  longer  than  i  c. 
The  rhythm  of  discharge  from  the  motor-cell,  as  far  as  the  un- 
dulations noted  indicate  rhythmic  response,  are  totally  different 
in  rhythm  from  that  of  the  action  induced  in  the  afferent  cell  by 
the  stimulation  applied.  In  the  reflex  centre  the  rhythm  has 
been  transmuted  from  one  rate  to  another.  Schafer  refers  this 
change  to  the  synapse. 

Again,  as  noted  above,  undulations  at  rates  varying  be- 
tween 7.5  and  12  per  second  are  seen  in  the  flexion-reflex  both 
in  its  after-discharge  and  during  the  excitation  and  quite  inde- 
pendently of  the  rate  of  delivery  of  the  induction  shocks  used 
as  stimuli  (Figs.  6  and  12),  and  even  when  the  stimulus  is  a 
constant  current.  Again,  reflexes  of  weak  intensity,  both  in  the 
case  of  the  flexion-reflex  and  of  the  crossed  extension-reflex, 
exhibit  at  their  commencement  a  stepped  form  of  myogram: 
the  steps  usually  succeed  each  other  at  about  eight  per  second, 
and  their  rate  is  independent  of  the  rate  of  delivery  of  the  electric 
stimuli  (Fig.  12)  exciting  the  reflex. 

In  such  cases,  therefore,  the  rhythm  of  the  end-effect  indi- 


44  THE   SIMPLE    REFLEX  [Lect. 


Figure  12.  —  Flexion-reflex  showing  imperfect  fusion  under  excitation  of  the  skin  with 
break  shocks  at  the  rate  of  10  per  second.  Signal  above  shows  the  six  interruptions  of 
the  primary  circuit.    Time  below  in  seconds. 

cates  that  in  transmission  along  the  reflex-arc  the  impulses 
generated  at  the  receptive  end  of  the  arc  are  not  actually  passed 
on  from  one  cell  element  to  another  in  the  arc,  but  that  new 
impulses  with  a  different  period  are  generated  in  the  course  of 
the  reflex-conduction.  This  is  confirmatory  of  a  neurone- 
threshold  as  a  feature  in  central  conduction. 

Refractory  phase.  A  conductor  which  replies  intermittently 
to  a  stimulus  exhibits  a  refractory  phase.  It  seems  that  even 
in  nerve-trunk  conduction  a  refractory  phase  must  occur;  other- 
wise, the  conductor  being  capable  of  reversible  direction  of  con- 
duction, a  backward  propagation  of  the  excited  state  as  well 
as  a  forward  would  ensue  from  every  point  of  the  conductor 
reached  by  the  nervous  impulse.  The  excited  state  would  then, 
when  once  excited,  maintain  itself  in  a  tetanic  manner  along  the 
whole  length  of  the  conductor.  The  propagation  would  thus 
lose  undulatory  character  such  as  we  know  it  has;  it  would 
merely  have  an  initial  wave-front.  But  this  refractory  phase  in 
nerve-fibres  seems  of  very  brief  duration,  not  longer  than  i  o-. 


II]  REFRACTORY   PHASE  45 

The  reflex-discharge  from  nerve-cells  seems  to  oe  rhythmic 
even  under  continuous  stimulation ;  but  the  cases  in  which  this 
has  been  examined  are  sparse.  The  existence  of  rhythm  in 
nerve-cell  discharge  is  presumptive  evidence  of  a  refractory 
phase  in  their  reaction.  Refractory  phase  was  first  called  atten- 
tion to  by  Kronecker  and  Stirling  ^^  in  1874,  in  the  heart,  and 
recognized  by  them  as  a  fact  of  central  importance  for  cardiac 
rhythm.  In  1876  Marey  ^'  met  the  same  phenomenon  and  gave 
it  the  name  by  which  it  is  now  known.  A  year  later  Romanes' 
fundamental  work  on  Medusa  demonstrated  the  existence  of  the 
same  phenomenon  there.^^  The  inconspicuous  duration  of  the 
phase  in  nerve-trunk  conduction  and  the  progress  of  the  view  that 
regards  the  heart  beat  as  of  myogenic  origin  have  contributed 
to  delay  recognition  of  refractory  phase  as  a  character  of  reflex- 
arc  reactions.  But  in  1899  Zwaardemaker  and  Lans^^®  showed 
the  supervention  of  a  refractory  phase  in  reflex  eyelid-closure. 
By  refractory  period  was  originally  meant  by  Marey  the  time 
during  which  the  heart  was  inexcitable  to  a  stimulus  however 
intense.  But  to-day  by  refractory  phase  is  understood  a  state 
during  which,  apart  from  fatigue,  the  mechanism  shows  less  than 
its  full  excitability.  The  cardiac  refractory  phase  is  absolute 
for  a  short  time  after  commencement  of  systole,  but  excitability 
then  returns  gradually.  In  the  eyelid-reflex  for  nearly  a  full 
second  after  initiation  of  a  reflex,  the  chance  that  a  second 
stimulus  then  delivered  will,  though  otherwise  appropriate,  ex- 
cite the  reflex,  is  fifty  per  cent  less  than  it  is  one  second  later. 
The  refractory  phase,  therefore,  is  marked  though  not  absolute : 
it  operates  longer  for  a  visual  stimulus  than  for  a  tactual  or 
thermal. 

Of  the  spinal  reflexes  of  the  dog's  hind  limb,  one,  namely,  the 
scratch-reflex,  shows  marked  refractory  phase.^^^  When  work- 
ing originally  with  this  reflex,  I  noticed  ^^^  that  the  rhythm  of 
the  scratching  movement  is  in  rate  independent  of  the  rhythm 
of  the  stimulus  evoking  it. 

In  the  dog,  when  the  spinal  cord  has  been  transected  in  the 
neck,  the  scratch  or  scalptor  reflex  becomes  in  a  few  months 
prominent.     A   stimulus  applied  at  any  point  within  a  large 


46 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect,    I 


Figure  13.  —  A.  The  "receptive  field,"  as  revealed  after  low  cervical  transection,  a  saddle- 
shaped  area  of  dorsal  skin,  whence  the  scratch-reflex  of  the  left  hind  limb  can  be  evoked. 
/r  marks  the  position  of  the  last  rib. 

B.  Diagram  of  the  spinal  arcs  involved.  L,  receptive  or  afferent  nerve-path  from 
the  left  foot ;  r,  receptive  nerve-path  from  the  opposite  foot ;  Ra,  r^,  receptive  nerve- 
paths  from  hairs  in  the  dorsal  skin  of  the  left  side;  FC,  the  final  common  path,  in  this 
case  the  motor  neurone  to  a  flexor  muscle  of  the  hip ;  Pa,  p/3,  proprio-spinal  neurones. 


saddle-shaped  field  of  skin  (Fig.  13)  excites  a  scratching  move- 
ment of  the  hind  leg.  The  movement  is  rhythmic  alternate 
flexion  and  extension  at  hip,  knee,  and  ankle.  Each  flexion 
recurs  at  a  frequency  of  about  four  times  per  second.  The 
stimuli  provocative  ^^  of  it  are  mechanical,  such  as  tickling  the 
skin  or  pulling  lightly  on  a  hair.     The  receptive  nerve-endings 


n] 


REFRACTORY   PHASE 


47 


which  generate  the  reflex  lie  in  the  surface  layer  of  the  skin, 
about  the  roots  of  the  hairs.  A  convenient  way  of  exciting  the 
reflex  is  by  feeble  faradization,  such  as  applied  to  one's  own 
tongue  is  felt  as  a  tickling  sensation.  For  exciting  the  reflex 
electrically,  I  place  a  broad  diffuse  electrode  on  some  indifferent 
part  of  the  surface  outside  the  receptive  field  of  skin,  and  apply 


Figure  14.  —  Tracing  of  the  flexion  of  the  hip  in  the  "  scratch-refiex  "  of  a  "  spinal  dog."  In 
A  the  reflex  is  evoked  by  lightly  rubbing  the  skin  at  a  point  behind  the  shoulder,  in  B  and 
C  by  unipolar  faradization  with  weak  double-induction  shocks  applied  to  the  same  point 
of  skin  through  a  needle  point  lightly  inserted  among  the  hair  roots.  Time  marked  in 
seconds  below.  At  the  top  in  B  and  C  an  electric  signal  marks  the  double-induction 
shocks  delivered,  and  at  the  bottom  an  electric  signal  marks  the  time  of  application  of  the 
stimulation. 


48 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


a  stigmatic  electrode  to  some  point  in  the  saddle-shaped  area  of 
dorsal  skin.  This  electrode  may  consist  of  a  minute  needle,  or 
a  gilt  entomological  pin ;  it  is  inserted  in  the  skin  so  lightly  that 
its  point  just  lies  among  the  hair  bulbs.  If  it  be  pushed  further, 
other  types  of  reflex  may  be  produced  and  the  scratch-reflex  be 
inhibited.  Prominent  among  the  muscles  active  in  this  reflex 
are  the  dorso-flexors  of  the  ankle,  the  flexors  of  the  knee,  and 
the  flexors  of  the  hip.  If  the  rhythm  of  the  last  is  recorded 
graphically,  tracings,*^  as  in  Figure  14,  are  obtained.  It  is  then 
demonstrable  that  the  series  of  brief  contractions  succeed  one 
another  at  a  rate  the  frequency  of  which  is  independent  of  that 
of  the  stimulation.  Thus,  the  rhythmic  reflex  is  elicitable  by  the 
application  of  a  heat-beam  or  a  constant  current.  The  make  and 
break  of  the  current  are  especially  able  to  excite  it  (Fig.  15),  but 
it  is  for  a  time  maintained  even  by  the  continued  passage  of  the 
current,  though  it  lapses  in  intensity  until  the  current  is  broken. 


Figure  15,  —  Scratch-reflex  evoked  by  a  galvanic  current  with  the  kathode  at  a  spot  of  skin 
behind  the  shoulder  and  a  diffuse  anode  applied  to  the  fore-paw.  The  make  of  the  current 
excites  a  stronger  reflex  than  the  break;  the  break  with  the  two  weaker  strengths  of 
current  Gi  and  G,  did  not  suffice  to  excite  the  reflex  at  all;  but  in  G^  it  did.  The 
signal  line  below  indicates  the  time  of  application  of  the  current.  Current  in  G,  was 
1.5  milleamperes. 


II]  REFRACTORY   PHASE  49 

when  it  appears  temporarily  with  renewed  vigor.  At  make  and 
break  of  the  voltaic  current  the  reflex-response  is  a  short  series 
of  rhythmic  flexions. 

The  reflex  is  still  more  easily  evoked  by  unipolar  application 
of  high  frequency  currents ;  it  can  be  evoked  with  great  vigour 
and  long  duration  by  this  mode  of  stimulation.  Double-induc- 
tion shocks  applied  at  frequencies  from  once  per  second  to  five 
hundred  and  twelve  times  per  second  and  at  various  intermedi- 
ate rates  all  evoke  it  easily;  so  likewise  do  single  break  or  make 
shocks  applied  at  rates  varying  in  my  experiments  between  1.33 
times  per  second  and  forty  times  per  second. 

Under  all  these  various  methods  of  excitation  (heat-beam, 
constant  current,  double  and  single  induced  currents,  high 
frequency  currents,  and  mechanical  stimuli)  the  rhythm  of  the 
flexor  response  remains  —  so  long  as  the  internal  conditions  of 
the  reflex  remain  unaltered  —  almost  the  same.^®'^  It  remains 
so  also  when,  instead  of  a  regular  succession,  a  grouped  succes- 
sion of  stimuli  is  used  for  excitation,  e.  g,,  stimuli  grouped  in 
twos  and  threes.  It  is  obvious  that  this  reflex  exhibits  a 
refractory  phase.  Take  the  instance  where  the  stimulus  applied 
consists  of  double-induction  shocks,  succeeding  each  other  at  a 
frequency  of  one  hundred  per  second.  The  reflex-arc  in  response 
produces  flexion  at  hip  about  four  times  per  second.  So  few 
as  three  successive  double-induction  shocks  will  suffice  to  excite 
the  reflex,  but  since  in  the  instance  taken  about  twenty  such 
shocks  correspond  in  time  with  each  flexor  beat,  and  each  such 
beat  is  divisible  into  about  equal  periods  of  contraction  and 
relaxation,  let  us  make  the  assumption  —  a  liberal  one  —  that 
ten  out  of  the  twenty  available  shocks  are  serving  as  stimuli. 
The  arc,  while  the  following  ten  are  being  applied,  fails  in  spite 
of  them  to  excite  the  flexor  muscles.  To  those  ten  it  does  not 
respond  at  all. 

Nor  can  this  refractory  state  be  overcome  by  simply  increas- 
ing the  intensity  of  the  stimuli.  The  reflex  remains  as  perfectly 
rhythmic  and  clonic  under  the  strongest  stimuli  as  under  weaker 
(Fig.  16).  The  frequency  of  the  beats  is  under  strong  stimula- 
tion often  somewhat  higher  than  under  quite  weak,  especially  at 

4 


i 

50  THE   SIMPLE  REFLEX  [Lect. 

outset  of  the  reflex,  but  the  difference  is  small,  e.  g,,  5.8  beats  per 
second,  instead  of  4.5  beats  per  second.  No  mode  or  inten- 
sity of  stimulation  to  which  I  have  had  recourse  converts  rhythmic 
clonic  beat  into  a  maintained  steady  contraction.  In  this  respect, 
as  in  certain  others,  the  scalptor  reflex-arc  closely  resembles  in 
its  behaviour  the  mechanism  of  the  Medusa  bell  and  heart-wall. 
It  resembles  these  more  closely  than  it  resembles  the  mamma- 
lian respiratory  reflex-arc,  which  can  under  several  circumstances 
be  made  to  produce  from  the  diaphragm  an  enduring  tetanic 
contraction. 

I  refer  to  the  contraction  of  the  flexor  muscles  in  the  scalptor- 
reflex  as  a  "  beat,"  not  implying  that  it  is  a  single  twitch,  but 
rather  because  the  term  denotes  a  short-lasting  phase  of  action 
in  a  rhythmic  series,  and  because  of  its  close  analogy  to  the 
beat  of  Medusa  and  of  the  heart. 

What  is  the  neuronic  construction  of  the  arc  in  the  scalptor- 
reflex?  The  reflex  is  in  a  sense  unilateral;  stimulation  of  the 
left  shoulder  evokes  scratching  by  the  left  leg,  not  the  right. 
Search  in  the  spinal  cord  ^^  for  the  paths  of  the  reflex  demon- 
strates that  a  lesion  breaking  through  one  lateral  half  of  the 
cord  anywhere  between  shoulder  and  leg  abolishes  the  ability 
of  the  skin  of  that  shoulder  to  excite  the  scratch-reflex,  but 
leaves  intact  the  reflex  of  the  opposite  shoulder.  In  the  lateral 
half  of  the  spinal  cord  which  the  reflex-path  descends,  severance 
of  the  dorsal  column  does  not  obviously  interfere  with  the 
reflex;  nor  does  the  severance  of  the  ventral  and  the  dorsal 
columns  together  of  that  side ;  no  more  does  severance  of  the 
gray  matter  in  addition.  But  severance  of  the  lateral  part  of 
the  lateral  column  itself  permanently  abolishes  the  conduction 
of  the  reflex ;  and  it  does  so  even  if  all  the  other  parts  of  the 
transverse  extent  of  the  cord  remain  intact.     The  paths  of  the 


Figure  16  (opposite).  —  Scratch-reflex,  provoked  by  42  break  shocks  delivered  at  the  rate  of  40 
per  second.  The  interruptions  of  primary  circuit  producing  these  are  recorded  by  electro- 
magnet giving  the  top  line  of  the  record.  As  the  stimulus  is  without  other  change  rendered 
more  and  more  intense  at  690,  11 00,  1900  and  3000  units  of  the  Kronecker  scale  respec- 
tively, the  reflexes  A  B  C  D  show  the  differences  seen.  Instead  of  one  beat  during  the 
stimulus  and  two  afterwards,  as  in  A,  the  reflex  gives  three  beats  during  the  stimulus  and 
six  beats  afterwards,  as  in  D.    Time  in  seconds  below. 


11] 


REFRACTORY   PHASE 


51 


s 


52  THE   SIMPLE  REFLEX  [Lect. 

reflex,  therefore,  descend  the  lateral  part  of  the  lateral  column. 
These  details  help  towards  construction  of  the  reflex-arc  in- 
volved. For  in  the  lateral  part  of  the  lateral  column,  as  shown 
by  the  method  of  successive  degeneration,  lie  long  proprio-spinal 
fibres  which  directly  connect  the  gray  matter  of  the  spinal  seg- 
ments of  the  shoulder  with  the  spinal  segments  containing  the 
motor  neurones  for  the  flexor  muscles  of  the  hip,  and  knee,  and 
ankle.  The  course  of  the  descending  proprio-spinal  fibres  can  be 
traced  and  their  number  counted.  The  method  of  "  successive 
degeneration  "  ^^^  enables  one  to  unravel  them  from  descending 
fibres  of  other  sources,  such  as  cerebral,  mesencephalic,  or  bul- 
bar, and  from  proprio-spinal  fibres  descending  from  the  foremost 
segments  of  the  neck.  This  is  done  —  as  the  term  **  successive 
degeneration"  implies  —  in  a  preHminary  lesion  by  severing 
that  part  of  the  spinal  cord  it  is  desired  to  examine  for  the  par- 
ticular proprio-spinal  fibres  sought,  from  all  the  central  nervous 
system  lying  farther  forward.  To  determine  the  proprio-spinal 
fibres  descending  from  the  third  and  fourth  thoracic  segments, 
the  first  step  is  to  transect  the  cord  between  the  second  and 
third  thoracic  segments.  There  then  ensues  throughout  the 
length  of  the  cord  behind  that  transection  degeneration  of  all 
the  fibres  that  enter  it  from  the  brain,  mid-brain,  bulb,  and  cer- 
vical and  first  two  thoracic  segments.  This  heavy  degeneration, 
after  developing,  reaches  a  maximum  and  gradually  passes  away, 
all  the  debris  of  the  degenerated  nerve-fibres  being  in  time 
removed.  For  this  a  period  of  a  year  suffices  in  a  dog.  The 
spinal  cord  is  then  ripe  for  determination  of  the  proprio-spinal 

Figure  \^  (opposite).  —  Cross-sections  of  the  spinal  cord  of  the  dog,  revealing  the  position  of 
the  nerve-tracts  descending  to  the  hind-limb  region  from  origin  in  the  foremost  three  thoracic 
segments,  by  the  method  of  "  successive  degeneration."  The  8th  cervical  segment  had  been 
exsected,  and  568  days  later  a  crosscut  was  made  at  the  hindmost  level  of  the  3d  thoracic 
s^ment.  The  transverse  extent  of  this  lesion,  as  determined  by  microscopical  sections 
afterwards,  is  shown  in  diagram  i  of  the  figure.  The  greater  part  of  the  right  lateral 
column  is  seen  to  have  been  spared  from  injury.  Three  weeks  subsequent  to  this  second 
lesion  the  animal  was  sacrificed.  Preparations  made  with  the  Marchi  method  for  reveal- 
ing degenerate  nerve-fibres  showed  the  degeneration  indicated  by  diagrams  2,  3,  4  and 
5  in  the  figure.  After  the  second  injury  to  the  cord  the  scratch-reflex  remained  elicitable 
from  the  right  shoulder,  but  was  lost  from  the  left  shoulder  in  its  anterior  scapular  region. 
The  degeneration  of  these  proprio-spinal  fibres  descending  from  the  shoulder  segments 
went,  therefore,  hand  in  hand  with  disappearance  of  the  scratch-reflex  from  a  region  of 
skin  of  the  shoulder  whence  it  was  elicitable  previously. 


i 


11] 


SUCCESSIVE   DEGENERATION 


53 


54  THE   SIMPLE  REFLEX  [Lect. 

fibres  it  is  desired  to  examine.  It  becomes  once  more  a  clean 
slate  on  which  a  new  degeneration  can  be  written.  The  proprio- 
spinal  fibres  are  revealed  by  making  a  transection  between  the 
fourth  and  fifth  thoracic  segments.  Four  weeks  after  this 
second  lesion  the  proprio-spinal  fibres  descending  from  the 
third  and  fourth  thoracic  segments  are  degenerate.  They  can 
be  studied  (Fig.  17)  throughout  their  course  from  the  fourth 
thoracic  segments  backwards  along  the  cord  by  any  of  the 
ordinary  methods,  such  as  the  Marchi  method,  for  studying  de- 
generate fibres.  Many  of  these  proprio-spinal  fibres  pass  from 
the  shoulder  segments  to  end  in  the  hind-limb  segments.  The 
existence  of  the  neuroglial  scar,  left  by  old  degeneration  that 
has  cleared  up,  far  from  complicating  the  tracing  of  the  new 
degeneration  assists  by  forming  a  contrast  background  to  it,  the 
sharpness  of  which  leaves  nothing  to  be  desired. 

From  the  results  obtained  by  this  method  the  following 
reflex-chain  can  be  inferred  ^^  as  possible  and  probable  for  the 
scratch-reflex. 

1.  The  receptive  neurone  (Fig.  13  B,  Ra)  from  the  skin  to  the 
spinal  gray  matter  of  the  corresponding  spinal  segment  in  the 
shoulder. 

2.  The  long  descending  proprio-spinal  neurone  (Fig.  13  B,  Pa) 
from  shoulder  segment  to  the  gray  matter  of  the  leg  segments. 

3.  The  motor  neurone  (Fig.  13  B,  F  C)  from  the  spinal  seg- 
ment of  the  leg  to  a  flexor  muscle. 

This  chain  thus  consists  of  three  neurones.  It  enters  the 
gray  matter  twice ;  that  is,  it  has  two  neuronic  junctions,  two 
synapses.     It  is  a  disynaptic  arc. 

In  venturing  to  thus  schematically  express  the  construction 
of  this  arc  as  disynaptic,  I  am  influenced  by  the  desire  to  ex- 
press its  construction  as  simply  as  possible  so  far  as  consistent 
with  the  ascertained  data  of  the  case.  I  therefore  omit  from 
the  scheme  the  possible  Schalt-Zellen  (v.  Monakow)  between 
Ra  and  Pa,  and  between  Pa  and  F  c.  Much  of  what  I  intend 
to  express  by  "  disynaptic  "  would  be  as  clearly  though  not 
as  concisely  expressed  by  saying  that  gray  matter  is  inter- 
calated  in   the  arc  twice  i.  e.,  at  two  separate  places.     That, 


II]  REFRACTORY  PHASE  55 

however,  is  not  expressible  by  a  single  adjective,  and  since 
synapses  occur  so  far  as  we  know  only  in  gray  matter,  disynap- 
tic  does  include  that  idea.  But  it  also  implies  somewhat  more. 
I  venture  upon  it  in  spite  of  the  assumptions  it  includes  because, 
in  my  opinion,  much  in  those  further  assumptions  seems  justi- 
fiable and  useful  as  a  working  hypothesis,  and  because  it  lays 
stress  on  the  importance  of  the  synapse  in  reflex  conduction,  — 
an  importance  which  for  reasons  given  before  seems  considerable. 

The  reflex-arc  consists,  therefore,  of  at  least  three  neurones. 
It  is  convenient  to  have  a  term  distinguishing  the  ultimate  neu- 
rone F  C  from  the  rest  of  the  arc.  For  reasons  to  be  given  later 
it  may  be  spoken  of  as  the  final  common  path^  The  rest  of 
the  arc  leading  up  to  the  final  common  path  is  conveniently 
termed  the  afferent  arc. 

The  morphological  components  of  this  reflex  mechanism 
include,  in  addition  to  the  above  neural  elements,  the  muscle- 
fibres  of  the  flexor  muscle  at  one  end,  and  possibly  a  receptive 
cutaneous  organ  at  the  other  end  probably  in  the  root-sheath  of 
a  hair.  Somewhere  in  this  chain  of  structures  the  property  of 
refractory  phase  has  its  seat,  and  refractory  phase  is  a  pivot  on 
which  the  whole  co-ordinating  mechanism  of  this  reflex  turns. 

In  attempting  to  locate  the  seat  of  the  refractory  state,  con- 
siderations that  arise  are  the  following.  The  muscle  involved  is 
one  which  neither  when  excited  directly  nor  through  its  motor- 
nerve  exhibits  this  refractory  period.  We  can  exclude  the 
phenomenon,  therefore,  from  it,  and  from  its  motor-nerve,  and 
from  the  link  between  it  and  its  motor-nerve,  —  the  end-plate. 
Further,  this  refractory  state  is  not  exhibited  when  the  motor 
neurones  of  this  muscle  are  excited  to  activity  by  various  other 
channels ;  for  instance,  by  the  afferent  neurones  coming  in  from 
the  receptive  organs  of  the  leg  itself,  or  by  the  palliospinal 
pyramidal  neurones  descending  to  them  from  the  cortex  of 
the  cerebral  hemisphere.  We  are  free,  therefore,  to  exclude  the 
motor  neurone  F  c  supplying  the  flexor  muscle  itself  as  the 
source  of  the  refractory  state  characterizing  the  scalptor-reflex. 
Again,  the  elicitation  of  the  reflex  typically  (Fig.  14)  by  all  the 
various  above-mentioned   forms  of  artificial  (electrical)  stimuli 


56  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

applied  through  a  needle  electrode  inserted  in  the  skin  suggests 
that  it  is  the  commencement  of  the  receptive  nerve-fibres  in  the 
skin  rather  than  any  specialized  cutaneous  sense-organ  therein 
that  are  the  seat  of  stimulation  when  the  reflex  is  thus  artificially 
excited.  We  have  no  knowledge  of  the  existence  of  a  refractory 
phase  of  such  duration  as  this  as  a  property  of  any  afferent  fibre 
passing  from  the  skin  to  the  spinal  cord.  There  is  indeed  con- 
clusive evidence  that  the  seat  of  this  refractory  phase  lies  neither 
in  the  skin  nor  in  the  afferent  neurone  itself.  When  the  reflex 
is  in  progress  under  stimulation  of  the  skin  at  one  point,  e.  g.,  Ka 
(Fig.  13,  B),  stimulation  at  some  other,  even  remote,  point  also 
producing  the  reflex  —  as  can  be  proved  in  various  ways  —  does 
not  break  the  rhythm  of  the  reflex  ^^s  300  or  complicate  it  in  any 
way.  The  reflex  elicited  from  spot  R/3  may  be  initiated  while  that 
elicited  from  Ra  is  in  progress,  and  may  then  be  carried  on  while  A 
is  allowed  to  cease,  or  vice  versa;  but  in  neither  case  is  the  rhythm 
of  the  reflex  broken  or  reduplicated.  And  this  although,  from  other 
evidence,  we  know  that  the  flexor  muscle  and  the  motor  neurone 
F  C  can  respond  rhythmically  to  stimuli  with  a  rate  of  rhythm 
more  than  twice  as  high  as  that  of  the  stimuli  applied  to  produce 
the  reflex.  Or  the  reflex  from  a  spot  B  may  be  introduced  into 
the  middle  of  one  from  a  spot  A,  and  although  its  reflex  (B) 
impresses  characters  of  its  own  as  to  amplitude,  direction  of  the 
foot,  etc.,  it  does  not  reduplicate  or  break  up  the  already  exist- 
ing rhythm.  The  result  does  not  differ  whether  the  individual 
stimuli  of  the  two  series  of  stimulations  at  spots  A  and  B  are 
alternate  or  not.  Figures  18,  19,  20,  21  illustrate  these  points. 
The  refractory  phases  obtaining  in  the  reflex  arising  from  spot 
A  are  respected  by  the  stimuli  delivered  at  B  also  and  are  not 
broken  down  by  them. 

There  is  evidently  some  part  of  the  reflex  mechanism  which 


Figure  18  (opposite).  —  Tracing  of  the  flexion  of  the  hip  in  the  "  scratch-reflex."  The  reflex  is 
evoked  by  two  separate  stimulations  (unipolar  faradization)  at  points  ten  centimeters  apart 
on  the  skin  surface.  The  upper  signal  shows  the  time  of  application  of  the  first  stimulation, 
and  the  line  immediately  below  that  the  frequency  of  repetition  of  the  double-induction 
shocks  of  that  stimulation.  The  lowest  line  signals  the  time  of  application  of  the  second 
stimulation  :  the  frequency  of  repetition  of  the  double  shocks  in  this  stimulation  was  much 
greater  than  in  the  other  stimulation  and  is  not  shown.     At  the  top  the  time  is  marked  in 


11] 


REFRACTORY   PHASE 


57  ^ 


fifths  of  seconds.  The  moment  of  commencement  of  the  first  stimulation  is  marked  by 
an  abscissa  on  the  base  line.  The  periods  of  the  two  separate  stimulations  overlap,  the 
second  beginning  a  full  second  before  the  first  ends,  but  no  interruption  or  increase  of  the 
rate  of  rhythmic  reflex-response  appears. 


53 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  19.  —  Similar  to  the  preceding,  but  with  different  points  of  stimulation  and  with  longer 
overlappmg  in  time  of  the  two  separate  stimulations. 


11] 


REFRACTORY   PHASE 


59 


A  B 

Figure  20.  —  Similar  to  the  preceding,  but  with  different  skin  points  and  slower  series  of  in- 
duction shocks,  the  moments  of  delivery  of  the  shocks  at  one  skin  point  being  midway  be- 
tween those  of  delivery  at  the  other.  The  sequence  of  the  stimulations  at  the  two  points 
in  B  is  the  reverse  of  that  in  A.  The  top  line  marks  the  time  in  fifths  of  seconds ;  the 
next  line  below  that  shows  the  frequency  of  repetition  of  the  weak  double  shocks,  the  rate 
being  the  same  for  both  stimuli ;  below,  the  two  signal  lines  showing  the  time  of  applica- 
tion of  each  of  the  two  separate  stimulations. 

is  common  to  impulses  started  both  at  A  and  at  B.     And  this 
holds  when  spots  A  and  B  lie  even  ten  centimetres  apart.     It  is 


6o 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 

A 


[Lect. 


Figure  21.  —  Records  of  scratch-reflex  as  before,  evoked  by  separate  unipolar  stimulation  of 
two  skin  points  8  centimeters  apart.  In  A  the  second  stimulation  commences  during  the 
after-discharge  of  the  first  reflex  and  causes  no  interruption  or  alteration  in  the  rhythm  of 
the  reflex-response.  In  B  the  second  stimulation  of  the  above  pair  evokes  the  reflex,  and 
during  its  progress  the  stimulation  which  comes  first  in  A  is  introduced  ;  and  conversely 
in  C.  In  none  of  the  three  cases  is  there  interruption  of  or  alteration  in  rate  of  the  rhythmic 
reflex-resp>onse.     Rate  of  delivery  of  the  exciting  shocks  is  shown  on  separate  lines. 

against  what  we  know  of  the  receptive  neurones  to  suppose  that 
there  is  between  them  any  collateral  nexus  beyond  their  impinge- 
ment directly  or  indirectly  on  other  neurones  more  or  less  common 
to  them  both.  The  seat  of  the  refractory  phase  seems  therefore 
to  lie  somewhere  central  to  the  receptive  neurones  in  the  affer- 
ent arcs.  The  refractory  phase  induced  in  some  element  of  the 
arc  by  the  reflex  from  A  extends  to  some  element  which  is  also 
concerned  in  the  conduction  of  the  reflex  induced  from  B.  This 
element  must  be  some  neurone  common  to  the  two  arcs  from  A 
andB  respectively.  Neurone  FC  (Fig.  13),  the  final  common  path, 
is  such  an  element.  But  neurone  FC  as  tested  by  other  reflexes, 
e.  g.,  the  flexion-reflex  shows  no  such  refractory  period.  The 
common  mechanism  sought  for  seems  therefore  to  lie  some- 
where between  FC  and  Ra,  R/3.    It  may  well  be  that  neurone  Pa 


II]      VALUE  OF  REFRACTORY  PHASE      6i 

is  partly  common  to  Ra  and  R/S,  for  these  R  neurones  are  well 
known  to  split  intraspinally  into  headward  and  tailward  stem- 
fibres,  each  carrying  many  collaterals,  and  probably  by  them  con- 
nected with  the  gray  matter  in  not  only  one  spinal  segment  but 
in  a  series  of  segments.  Collaterals  from  Rff  as  well  as  from 
Ra  may  reach  Pa,  therefore ;  and  similarly  with  Py3. 

The  scratch-reflex  has  instructive  points  of  likeness  to  that 
of  the  swimming-beat  of  Medusa.  The  arrangement  of  its 
response  is  quite  like  that  of  the  muscular  response  of  the 
swimming-bell  of  Medusa  under  stimulation  of  two  points  of 
the  subumbrella  or  two  of  the  marginal  receptor  organs.  We 
can  compare  each  lateral  half  of  the  saddle-shaped  receptive 
area  of  the  dog's  back  in  regard  to  the  scratch-reflex  quite 
strictly  to  the  marginal  surface  of  Rhizostoma  in  regard  to  its 
swimming-beat  reflex. 

In  Medusa  a  second  stimulus  following  close  upon  a  first 
does  not  prolong  the  contraction ;  ^^  it  finds  the  bell  in  refractory 
phase.  The  beat  induced  by  the  first  stimulus  has  no  second 
contraction  fused  with  it  in  consequence  of  the  second  stimulus. 
The  principles  of  the  co-ordination  thus  obtained  in  the  rela- 
tively simple  swimming  of  Medusa  seem  as  follows. 

One  condition  of  the  co-ordination  of  the  swimming-beat, 
as  said  above,  is  the  unpolarized  nature  of  the  nerve-channels 
allowing  free  flow  of  nervous  impulses  in  either  direction  from 
conductor  to  conductor  along  the  nerve-net.  Another  condition 
is  the  continuity  mediate  or  immediate  of  every  conductor  with 
every  other.  These  conditions  of  the  nervous  system  allow  a 
single  stimulus  given  at  any  single  point  to  evoke  a  co-ordinate 
contraction  of  the  whole  musculature,  to  evoke,  in  short,  a  full 
and  perfect  swimming  stroke  or  beat.  But  they  do  not  insure 
that  under  a  series  of  stimuli  delivered  in  irregular  and  varied 
sequence  and  at  various  points  in  perhaps  rapid  succession  a 
series  of  co-ordinate  strokes  or  beats  shall  result. 

Romanes  '^  showed  that  the  receptor  organs  at  the  edge  of 
the  bell  were  the  source  of  the  natural  beats  of  the  bell,  and  that 
so  long  as  even  one  of  these  remained  the  swimming-bell  con- 
tinued to  beat  spontaneously.     There  therefore  exist  in  this  case 


62  THE  SIMPLE  REFLEX  [Lect. 

quite  a  number  of  points  which  all  tend  under  natural  circum- 
stances to  initiate  the  beats  of  the  bell. 

Suppose  that  shortly  after  a  stimulus  has  occurred  at  A,  and 
before  the  contraction  induced  by  it  has  passed  off,  another 
stimulus  is  delivered  at  B,  and  then  similarly  at  C.  All  these 
stimuli  are,  taken  singly,  similarly  provocative  of  locomotion, 
and  the  only  locomotive  act  of  the  creature  is  the  systolic  stroke 
of  its  bell.  Each  reaction  is  therefore  aimed  at  incitement  of 
locomotion  only  for  a  propulsive  movement  of  the  bell.  But 
the  two  conditions,  /.  ^.,  unpolarized  conduction  and  end-to- 
end  continuity  obtaining  in  the  nerve-net,  though  they  insure 
that  result  for  one  stimulus,  defeat  it  in  the  case  of  a  series  of 
stimuli  quickly  following  either  at  one  and  the  same  point  or  at 
several,  —  and  for  the  following  reason.  The  series  of  stimuli 
would,  were  those  two  conditions  all,  merely  immobilize  the 
bell.  The  second  stimulus  following  during  the  contraction 
excited  by  the  first  would  be  conducted,  as  was  the  former,  to 
all  the  musculature  of  the  movement,  and  would  simply  accentu- 
ate and  prolong  the  systolic  condition  already  in  progress.  But 
that  would  interrupt  locomotion,  not  promote  it.  The  con- 
traction of  the  muscular  disc  to  produce  the  stroke  must  be 
immediately  preceded  by  diastole,  enabling  it  to  embrace  the 
proper  volume  of  water  for  re-expulsion. 

An  essential  feature  of  the  co-ordination  is  therefore  due  to 
alternation  of  the  two  converse  phases  of  contraction  and  relax- 
ation, in  the  one  muscle;  these  execute  the  locomotion  of  this 
simple  animal.  This  necessary  condition  is  insured,  even  under 
irregular  series  of  stimuli,  by  refractory  phase. 

When  the  bell  is  replying  or  has  just  replied  to  a  stimulus,  it 
remains  inexcitable  to  further  stimuli  for  a  period  which  outlasts 
its  phase  of  contraction.  This  prevents  disharmony  occurring 
in  the  rhythmic  movement  under  multiple  stimulation. 

It  is  on  conditions  like  these  governing  Medusa's  swimming- 
bell  that  the  co-ordination  of  the  heart's  action  is  based,  except 
that  in  the  heart  there  is  but  one  initiatory  spot,  prepotent  per- 
manently, as  in  Vertebrates,  or  temporarily,  as  in  Tunicates.  In 
Medusa  presumably  any  one  of  various  specialized  points  (border 


II]      PERIPHERAL,   CENTRAL   CO-ORDINATION       63 

organs)  becomes  prepotent  at  different  times,  by  reason  of  stimu- 
lation from  the  environment.  In  this  Medusa  more  closely  re- 
sembles the  scratch-reflex,  where  any  one  of  many  points  in  a 
large  receptive  field  becomes  temporarily  prepotent  under  stim- 
ulation, e.  g.,  puncture  by  a  flea  or  other  parasite,  and  then 
initiates  and  leads  a  series  of  beats  which  can  be  prolonged 
or  intensified  by  concurrent  stimulation  by  parasites  at  other 
points,  but  cannot  by  their  concurrence  be  upset  as  regards 
rhythm.  In  the  action  of  scratching  it  is  as  necessary  as  in  the 
swimming  of  Medusa,  or  the  beating  of  the  heart,  that  relaxa- 
tion follow  contraction. 

Refractory  phase  is  obviously  an  essential  condition  in  the 
co-ordination  of  the  scalptor-reflex.  The  scratching-reflex,  in 
order  to  secure  its  aim,  must  evidently  consist  of  a  succession 
of  movements  repeated  in  the  same  direction,  and  intervening 
between  the  several  members  of  that  series  there  must  be  a 
complemental  series  of  movements  in  the  opposite  direction. 
Whether  these  two  series  involve  reflex  contractions  of  two 
antagonistic  muscle-groups  respectively  in  alternate  time,  I  would 
leave  for  the  present.  The  muscle-groups  or  their  reflex-arcs 
must  show  phases  of  refractory  state  during  which  stimuli  can- 
not excite,  alternating  with  phases  in  which  such  stimuli  easily 
excite.  Evidently  this  is  fundamental  for  securing  return  to  the 
initial  position  whence  the  next  stroke  shall  start.  The  refrac- 
tory phase  secures  this.  By  its  extension  through  the  whole 
series  of  arcs  it  prevents  that  confusion  which  would  result  were 
refractory  phase  in  some  of  the  arcs  allowed  to  concur  with  ex- 
citatory phase  in  others. 

But  there  is  one  significant  difference  between  refractory 
state  in  the  scratch-reflex  and  in  the  swimming  mechanism  of 
Medusa.  In  the  latter,  as  in  the  heart,  the  refractory  state  is  a 
property  not  relegated  to  a  central  nervous  organ  remote  from 
the  peripheral  tissue  in  whose  function  it  finds  expression.  It 
is  located  in  intimate  connection  with  the  peripheral  organ  itself. 
From  observations  of  Bethe  it  seems  likely  that  refractory  phase 
in  Medusa  is  a  function  of  the  nerve-net.  Magnus^®*  has  re- 
cently shown  that  the  refractory  phase  of  the  beat  of  the  isolated 


I 

"I 

64  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect,  ; 

intestine  is  referable  to  the  local  nerve-plexus  (Auerbach's)  lying  [ 
in  the  gut  wall.  In  these  cases  the  refractory  state  seems  to  ,: 
belong  to  the  nervous  elements,  but  to  nervous  elements  diffused  | 
through  the  peripheral  tissue.  But  in  the  scratch-reflex  the  | 
site  of  the  refractory  state  is  central,  intraspinal.  | 

The  centrality  of  seat  of  the  refractory  state  of  the  scratch-  | 
reflex  is  significant  of  the  difference  of  the  conditions  under  which  | 
the  scratch-reflex  and  the  swimming  of  Medusa  respectively  go  5 
forward.     In  the  case  of  the  locomotor  action  of  the  swimming-  I 
bell  of  Medusa,  we  have  a  simple  musculature  which  can  execute  '] 
practically  but  one  movement     It  is  in  fact  a  single  muscle,  that  ;j 
is  to  say,  comparable  with  what  in  the  more  complex  muscula-  I 
ture  of  higher  organisms  —  e.  g.^  vertebrates  —  is  regarded  as  a    i 
unit  of  musculature,  a  single  muscle,  such  as  the  gastrocnernuis,   \ 
tibialis,  etc.,  in  the  frog.      Each  and  every  receptor  organ  which    ■ 
under  stimulation  produces  locomotion  is  therefore  connected  by 
nerve  with  that  single  muscle  of  locomotion,  and  when  impelled 
by  each  or  any  of  them,  the  muscle  effects  practically  the  same 
action  as  it  does  when  impelled  by  any  other  of  the  sister  receptor 
organs.    The  movement  of  locomotion  which  is  provoked  through 
each  receptor  is  practically  the  same  as  that  provoked  through 
any  of  the  rest.    The  mechanical  organ  in  this  case  can  perform 
but  one  movement,  and  its  performance  of  that  movement  is, 
so  to  say,  the  one  purpose  demanded  from  it  by  each  of  all  the 
receptor  channels  playing  upon  it. 

But  with  the  mechanical  organ  which  the  scratch-reflex 
employs  the  case  is  different.  That  organ  is  the  hind  limb,  a 
complex  structure  built  of  parts,  many  of  them  spatially  op- 
posed, and  able  as  a  whole  to  execute  movements  of  various 
kinds.  Thus  it  can  reflexly  not  only  scratch,  but  stand,  walk, 
run,  or  gallop,  squat  in  defaecation,  abduct  and  flex  in  mic- 
turition, etc.  In  the  swimming-bell  of  Medusa  there  is  no 
opportunity  for  antagonism  between  the  motor  end-results  of 
the  reflexes  that  employ  it,  save  in  respect  to  the  possible 
confusion  of  successive  contractions  which  would  destroy  the 
rhythmic  pulse,  and  that  confusion  is  avoided  by  refractory 
phase.     The  swimming-bell  of  Medusa  is  at  the  behest  of  but 


II]       SEAT  OF  REFRACTORY  PHASE       65 

one  type-reflex.  The  scratch-reflex  possesses  the  same  safe- 
guard against  destruction  of  its  rhythmic  character.  But  in  the 
case  of  the  scratch-reflex  that  reflex  is  but  one  of  several  re- 
flexes that  share  in  a  condominium  over  the  efl"ector  organ  — 
the  limb.  It  must  therefore  be-  possible  for  the  scratch-reflex, 
taken  as  a  whole,  to  be,  as  occasion  demands,  replaced  in  exer- 
cise of  its  use  of  the  limb  by  other  reflexes,  and  many  of  these 
do  not  require  clonic  action  from  the  limb,  —  indeed,  would 
be  defeated  by  clonic  action.  It  would  not  do,  then,  for  the 
peripheral  organ  itself  to  be  a  clonic  mechanism.  The  clonic 
mechanism  must  lie  at  some  place  where  other  kinds  of  reflex 
can  preclude  the  clonic  actuator  from  affecting  the  peripheral 
organ.  Now  such  a  place  is  obviously  the  central  organ  itself; 
for  that  organ  is,  as  its  name  implies,  a  nodal  point  of  meeting 
to  which  converge  all  the  nervous  arcs  of  the  body,  and  among 
others  all  those  which  for  their  several  ends  have  to  employ  the 
same  mechanical  organ  as  does  the  scratch-reflex  itself.  It  is 
therefore  only  in  accord  with  expectation  that  the  seat  of  the 
refractory  phase  of  the  scratch-reflex  lies  where  we  traced  it,  in 
the  central  nervous  organ  itself,  and  somewhere  between  the 
motor  neurone  to  the  muscle  and  the  receptive  neurone  from  the 
skin.    For  it  is  upon  the  motor  neurone  that  other  arcs  impinge. 

The  refractory  state  is  obviously  akin  to  a  state  of  inhibition, 
and  just  as  there  are  well-known  examples  in  which  the  inhibi- 
tory state  is  peripheral  (e.g.t  the  heart),  and  others  in  which  the 
inhibition  is  central,  so  undoubtedly  phases  of  refractory  state 
are  in  some  instances  peripheral,  but  also  in  numerous  instances 
are  central,  —  and  this  is  so  in  certain  reflex  actions. 

The  reflexes  of  which  refractory  phase  constitutes  a  promi- 
nent feature  are  those  concerned  with  cyclic  actions  occurring 
in  rhythmic  series ;  such  as  the  scratch-reflex,  reflexes  of  swal- 
lowing and  blinking,  and  probably  the  rhythmically  recurring 
reflexes  concerned  in  the  stepping  of  the  Hmbs. 

Nothnagel  (1870),^*  following  up  an  observation  by  Sets- 
chenow,^^  studied  a  periodic  and  rhythmic  reflex  of  the  crossed 
hind  limb  in  the  spinal  frog.  He  found  that  if  some  days  after 
the  frog's  cord  had  been  transected  at  the  fourth  vertebra,  the 

5 


e^  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

central  end  of  the  sciatic  is  faradized,  a  rhythmic  alternating 
flexion  and  extension  of  the  opposite  hind  limb  is  evoked.  He 
noted  that  no  intensity  of  stimulation  makes  the  rhythmic  move- 
ment alter  from  clonic  to  tonic.  A  reflex  of  this  kind  is,  I 
find,  elicitable  in  the  spinal  dog's  hind  leg  by  unipolar  faradiza- 
tion of  the  opposite  foot,  and,  as  Nothnagel  noted  in  the  frog, 
no  mere  increase  of  intensity  of  stimulation  converts  its  clonic 
character  into  maintained  tonic.  It  is  rhythmic  (Fig.  22),  and 
has  a  refractory  period  which  no  ordinary  increase  of  inten- 
sity of  stimulation  suffices  to  break  down.  The  frequency  of  its 
rhythm  averages  2.3  per  second,  but  varies  somewhat  in  different 
observations.  This  is  about  twice  as  slow  as  the  frequency  of 
the  scratch-reflex,  which  averages  4.5  per  second.  The  average 
rhythm  of  the  scratch-reflex  is  almost  exactly  the  same  as  that 
ascertained  by  Gotch  and  Burch  ^^*  for  reflex-discharge  from  i 
the  electric  cell  of  Malapterurus,  but  the  rate  in  Malapterurus  I 
appears  to  vary  much  more  than  that  of  the  scratch-reflex.  >; 


Figure  22.  —  Crossed  stepping-reflex  (spinal  dog)  elicited  by  unipolar  faradization  of  the 
opposite  foot.  The  flexions  of  the  limb  succeed  each  other  at  a  rate  of  about  five  times  in 
two  seconds  —  about  half  the  frequence  they  exhibit  in  the  scratch-reflex.  The  "after- 
discharge"  of  the  reflex  includes  four  "steps,"  the  last  being  of  small  amplitude  and 
slow,  as  with  the  final  beats  of  the  scratch-reflex.  Time  marked  below  in  seconds.  Above 
the  time  line  is  the  signal  line,  showing  the  period  of  application  of  the  stimulus. 


IIJ 


THE   STEPPING-REFLEX 


67 


^IGURK  23.  —  The  "  extensor-thrust  *' ;  spinal  dog.    Time  below  in  seconds. 


There  is  a  peculiar  brief  extension-reflex  of  the  dog's  hind 
leg  which  I  term^  the  "extensor-thrust"  Baglioni  288  has 
more  recently  noted  an  analogous  reflex  in  the  frog.  This 
reflex  is  elicited  by  mechanical  stimuli  applied  to  the  planta. 
In  the  spinal  dog,  where  well  marked,  it  is  often  elicitable  by 
even  lightly  stroking  with  the  edge  of  a  piece  of  paper  the  skin 
behind  the  plantar  cushion.  It  is  more  certainly  evoked  by 
pushing  the  finger-tip  between  the  plantar  cushion  and  the  toe- 
pads,  especially  when  the  hip  and  knee,  not  necessarily  the 
ankle,  are  resting,  passively  flexed. 

The  **  extensor-thrust "  I  have  never  succeeded  in  prolonging 
to  a  full  half-second  —  none  of  my  records  exhibit  it  as  long  as 
that  Usually  the  duration  as  recorded  is  about  a  fifth  of  a  second 
(Fig.  23.).  Possibly  its  muscular  contraction  may  be  a  simple 
twitch,  though  reflexly  excited.  Its  muscular  field  involves  the 
muscles  of  the  "  knee-jerk."  The  myogram  of  the  extensor 
thrust  is  shorter  than  that  of  tetanic  contraction  of  the  dia- 
phragm (Head's  slip)  ^^^  caused  by  two  successive  stimuli  to  the 


68 


THE   SIMPLE    REFLEX 


[Lect. 


phrenic  nerve  when  the  muscular  responses  of  the  two  just  fuse    J 
(Fig.  24). 

Immediately  after  its  elicitation  this  reflex,  in  my  experience,    f 
remains  in  the  spinal  dog  for  nearly  a  second  relatively  inelicit- 


FiGURB  24. —  Myogram  of  the  contraction  of  the  diaphragm  of  the  rabbit  (Head's  slip) 
elicited  by  two  break  shocks  applied  to  the  phrenic  nerve.  The  moments  of  application  of 
the  two  shocks  are  indicated  by  abscissae  on  the  myograph  curve.  Time  below  in  hun- 
dredths of  a  second  (Macdonald  and  Sherrington). 


able.  Its  reflex-arc  exhibits  after  its  phase  of  activity  a  refrac- 
tory phase.  The  refractory  phase  is  here  far  longer  than  that 
of  the  scalptor-reflex.  It  may  last  six  times  as  long  as  the 
period  of  activity ;  thus  the  extensor-thrust  may  last  only  1 70  a 
while  the  succeeding  refractory  phase  may  endure  a  full  second. 
The  extensor-thrust  is  probably  an  important  element  in  the 
reflex  mechanism  of  the  dog's  locomotion.^  One  peculiarity  it 
has  as  compared  with  other  spinal  reflexes  of  the  limb  is  the 
considerable  force  which  it  exerts.  In  the  locomotion  of  the 
animal  it  provides  much  of  the  propulsive  power  required. 
1  Compare  on  this  the  recent  paper  by  M.  Philippson.^**® 


II]  THE   EXTENSOR-THRUST  69 

Bearing  these  points  in  mind,  it  is  obvious  that  as  an  element  in 
locomotion  its  repetition  is  required  only  at  intervals  consider- 
ably longer  than  the  duration  of  the  thrust  itself,  namely,  at  a 
particular  phase  of  each  successive  step  taken  in  the  progression 
of  the  animal.  After  the  extensor-thrust,  the  limb  has  to  be 
given  over  to  the  flexor  muscles  in  order,  without  touching  the 
ground,  to  swing  forward  in  preparation  for  the  next  step  by  the 
limb.  It  is  reasonable  to  suppose  that  part  of  the  means  by 
which  selective  adaptation  has  secured  this  result  is  the  evolu- 
tion of  the  long  refractory  phase  following  the  activity  in  the 
reflex-arc  of  the  extensor-thrust.  Zwaardemaker  ^^^  has  shown 
that  the  reflex  movement  of  swallowing  in  the  narcotized  cat  is 
followed  by  a  refractory  period,^  lasting  half  a  second  or  longer. 
This  refractory  state  is  central,  for  when  the  reflex  swallow  has 
been  elicited  by  the  superior  laryngeal  nerve  of  one  side,  the 
after-lasting  refractory  period  holds  good  also  for  excitation  of 
the  opposite  superior  laryngeal  nerve. 

Variation  of  the  external  stimulus  has  comparatively  little 
eflect  upon  the  length  of  the  refractory  period.  But  internal 
conditions,  such  as  blood  supply,  fatigue,  narcosis,  etc.,  do 
influence  it  greatly.  For  reflexes  which  exhibit  a  refractory 
phase,  a  certain  duration  of  that  phase,  subject  to  some  varia- 
tions, is  characteristic.  The  duration  of  the  phase  varies  con- 
siderably in  different  types  of  these  reflexes. 

It  is  clear  that  an  essential  part  of  many  reflexes  is  a  more  or 
less  prolonged  refractory  phase  succeeding  nervous  discharge. 

Refractory  phase  appears  therefore  at  the  one  end  and  at  the 
other  of  the  animal  scale  as  a  factor  of  fundamental  importance 
in  the  co-ordination  of  certain  motile  actions.  In  the  lowly 
animal  form  (Medusa)  it  attaches  locally  to  the  neuro-muscular 
organ,  and  so  also  in  the  visceral  and  blood-vascular  tubes 
of  Vertebrates.  But  in  higher  forms  (dog)  refractory  phase 
occurs  as  regards  the  taxis  of  the  skeletal  musculature,  not  in 
the  peripheral  neuro-muscular  organ,  but  in  the  centres  of  the 
nervous  system  itself. 

^  Baglioni  has  pointed  out  refractory  phase  in  a  reflex  in  the  frog.*** 


70  THE   SIMPLE  REFLEX  [Lect. 


LECTURE    III 

CO-ORDINATION   IN   THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  {concluded) 

Argument :  Correspondence  between  intensity  of  stimulus  and  intensity 
of  reflex  reaction.  Differences  between  different  reflexes  in  this  re- 
spect. Functional  solidarity  of  the  intraspinal  group  of  elements 
composing  a  reflex  "centre."  Sensitivity  of  reflexes,  as  compared 
with  nerve-trunks,  to  asphyxial  and  anaemic  conditions,  and  to  an- 
aesthetic and  certain  other  drugs.  Functional  significance  of  the 
neural  perikarya.  Reflexes  of  double-sign.  Reflexes  of  successive 
double-sign,  and  of  simultaneous  double-sign.  Evidence  of  recipro- 
cal innervation  in  reflexes.  Reflex  inhibition  of  the  tonus  of  skeletal 
muscles.  Reflex  inhibition  of  the  knee-jerk.  Time-relations  and 
other  characters  of  reflex  inhibition  as  exemplified  by  the  flexion- 
reflex.  Other  examples  of  inhibition  as  part  of  reflex  reciprocal 
innervation.  The  seat  of  this  reflex  inhibition  is  intraspinal.  Conver- 
sion of  reflex  inhibition  into  reflex  excitation  by  strychnine  and  by 
tetanus  toxin.  Significance  of  the  "  central  "  situation  of  reflex  in- 
hibition in  the  cases  here  dealt  with. 

Grading  of  intensity.  A  further  diflference  between  the  re- 
action of  a  reflex-arc  and  that  of  a  nerve-trunk  lies  in  the 
greater  ease  with  which  in  the  latter  the  intensity  of  effect  can 
be  graded  by  grading  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus.  In  the 
nerve-trunk  this  has  been  examined  both  with  the  action-current 
(Waller)  ^^  and  for  motor-nerves  by  the  muscular  contraction 
(Pick,  Cybulski,  and  Zanietowski).^*°  The  accuracy  of  grading 
within  a  certain  range  of  stimulus-intensity  is  so  remarkable 
that  the  ratio  between  stimulus-intensity  and  response-intensity 
has  by  some  observers  been  assigned  mathematical  expression. 
Waller  finds  the  response  in  a  nerve-trunk,  directly  stimulated, 
increase  in  much  closer  direct  proportion  to  the  increment  of 
external  stimulus  than  does  the  response  of  muscle  to  indirect 
stimulation,  or  the  response  of  the  optic  nerve  when  the  retina 
is  adequately  stimulated.  The  correspondence  between  inten- 
sity of  external   stimulus  and   reflex  end-effect  is   again   less 


Ill]        GRADING   OF   INTENSITY  OF  REFLEX  71 

close  still;  indeed  it  is  often  stated  that  reflex  reactions  re- 
semble as  to  intensity  the  "  all-or-nothing "  principle  of  the 
cardiac  beat  (Wundt).  Biedermann  remarks  of  reflexes  evoked 
by  single-induction  shocks  in  the  cooled  frog,  that  there  is 
practically  no  grading  of  intensity:  they  are  all  maximal.  Bag- 
lioni  makes  the  same  remark  for  other  reflexes  in  the  frog. 

Yet  graded  intensity  of  reflex-effect  does  occur.  Walton  ^^ 
noted  as  one  of  the  features  of  strychnine  poisoning,  that  at 
a  certain  stage  the  grading  of  intensity  is  lost  and  all  reflexes 
become  maximal.  Merzbacher  ^^^  and  Pari^^  have  supplied 
some  measurements  of  the  increment  in  amplitude  of  the  reflex 
movements  of  the  frog's  leg  under  increase  of  intensity  of 
stimulation. 

The  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  of  the  spinal  dog  in- 
creases in  amplitude  in  correspondence  with  increase  of  intensity 
of  stimulus  —  alterations  of  time-relations  of  stimulus  being  ex- 
cluded. Increase  of  intensity  of  stimulus  heightens  the  reflex 
contraction  both  in  power  and  amplitude.  Figure  25  shows  a 
successive  series  of  these  reflexes,  each  elicited  by  a  series  of 
break  shocks  delivered  at  the  same  skin-spot  by  a  stigmatic 
kathode.  The  increments  of  the  reflex  run  fairly  steadily  with 
the  up-gradient  of  intensity  of  stimulus. 

In  the  scratch-reflex  a  grading  of  the  intensity  of  the  re- 
flex is  easily  obtainable  by  grading  the  intensity  of  the  stimu- 
lus. By  a  suitably  weak  stimulus  a  scratch-reflex  can  be 
elicited  which  exhibits  but  a  single  beat.  Increase  of  intensity 
of  the  reaction  does  not  show  itself  in  increase  in  frequency  of 
the  rhythm  of  this  reflex,  or  shows  itself  very  slightly  in  that 
form,  the  refractory  period  being  hardly  curtailed  at  all.  The 
increase  reveals  itself  as  greater  amplitude  of  the  individual  beats 
of  the  rhythmic  contraction.  By  simply  bringing  the  secondary 
coil  nearer  to  the  primary  in  a  dozen  successive  steps,  it  is  easy 
to  obtain  a  dozen  grades  of  amplitude  in  a  dozen  successive 
examples  of  this  reflex  (Fig.  26).  The  beats  in  response  to  a 
strong  stimulus  may  have  six  times  the  amplitude  of  those  evoked 
by  a  weak.  The  single  beat  that  can  be  obtained  by  a  suitably 
feeble  stimulus  (Fig.  9)  is  not  only  small  but  slow ;  it  resembles 


^^  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

the  last  beat  the  reflex  gives  as  it  dies  out  after  cessation  of  an 
ordinary  stimulus.  The  feeble,  slow  character  of  the  terminal 
beat  of  the  ordinary  reflex  is  not  therefore  due  to  fatigue,  but 
simply  to  weak  intensity  of  excitatory  process  at  the  moment. 

The  scratch-reflex,  though  it  resembles  the  heart  beat  in 
relative  immutability  of  rhythm  under  change  of  intensity  of 
stimulation,  differs  from  it  in  the  change  of  intensity  of  its  beat, 
which  follows  change  in  intensity  of  stimulus.  It  does  not  ob- 
serve the  "  all-or-nothing  "  principle.  It  is  obvious  that  in  the 
heart  beat  the  object  is  to  put  a  pressure  on  the  contents  of 
the  ventricle  higher  than  that  obtaining  in  the  aorta,  and  that 
aim  reached,  any  further  excess  of  pressure  is  useless  or  harmful, 
for  it  subjects  the  heart  and  the  arterial  wall  to  an  unnecessary 
strain.  Clifford  Allbutt  ^^  remarks :  "  It  is  the  function  of  a 
healthy  heart  and  arteries  to  promote  the  maximum  of  blood 
displacement  with  the  minimal  alteration  of  pressures."  The 
stress  under  which  the  heart  is  driven  is  less  closely  associated 
with  intensities  of  stimulus  than  with  conditions  internal  to  itself, 
e.  g.y  distension,  etc.  But  with  the  scratching  movement  it  is 
obvious  that  a  strong  scratching  movement  may  remove  an  irrita- 
tion more  quickly  and  more  effectually  than  weak  movement. 

On  the  crossed  extension-reflex  the  effect  of  increase  of  in- 
tensity of  stimulus  shows,  in  my  experience,  somewhat  differently. 
After  a  relatively  slow  and  gradual  increase  of  reflex-response, 
there  appears  at  a  certain  intensity  of  stimulation  a  sudden  rela- 
tively large  increase   of  the  response.     This  augmentation    is 

Figure  25.  —  The  flexion-reflex,  showing  gradient  of  intensity  due  to  graded  intensities  of 
stimulus.  The  time  of  the  stimulus  is  marked  by  the  signal  line  above ;  the  stimulus  con- 
sisted in  each  case  of  12  break  shocks  delivered  at  the  rate  of  25  per  second,  applied  by  uni- 
polar method  with  kathode  needle  point  in  skin  of  a  digit,  the  diffuse  pole  lying  headward 
of  the  spinal  transection.  The  interval  between  commencement  of  each  reflex  was  two 
minutes. 

Intensity  ofstimului.  Measure  of  reflex 

A.  690  8.5 

B.  3000  59 

C.  5200  no 
D  9800  168 
E.  12500  213 
B,.  3000  29 

Intensity  is  given  in  units  of  the  Kronecker  scale.     Time  marked  below  in  seconds 
From  same  animal  as  yielded  observations  of  Fig.  44  but  on  the  succeeding  day. 


Ill]       GRADING  OF   INTENSITY   OF   REFLEX  73 


74  THE   SIMPLE    REFLEX  [Lect. 

chiefly  in  the  form  of  "  after-discharge  "  (Fig.  27).  This  reflex 
shows  well  that  internal  conditions  play  a  greater  r6le  as  com- 
pared with  external  in  reflex  conductions  than  in  nerve-trunk 
conduction.  Under  strong  stimuli  the  augmentation  of  the 
reaction  by  after-discharge  becomes  enormous. 

The  "  extensor-thrust "  I  have  failed  to  evoke  by  any  stimulus 
easy  to  grade  or  record  a  measure  of  But  my  experience  of  it 
under  the  particular  form  of  mechanical  stimulation  it  appears 
to  require  leads  me  to  think  that  strength  of  external  stimulus 
affects  the  reflex-response  but  little,  and  that  this  reflex  does 
much  resemble  the  heart  in  responding  either  not  at  all  or  fully. 
Its  graphic  record  time  after  time  in  a  series  of  stimulations  re- 
peats itself  with  very  little  diff'erence  of  character. 

Therefore,  from  the  reflexes  of  the  limb  of  the  spinal  dog,  it 
would  appear  that  in  respect  to  ability  to  be  graded  in  intensity 
in  accordance  with  grading  of  intensity  of  stimulus,  there  exist 
great  differences  between  the  various  type-reflexes.  Some  re- 
flexes—  e.g.y  the  flexion-reflex  and  the  scratch-reflex  —  easily 
exhibit  grading,  while  others  do  not.  The  difference  between 
reflex-conduction  in  various  reflexes  in  this  respect  may  explain 
the  discrepancies  between  various  observers  on  this  point. 

A  factor  in  the  grading  of  submaximal  effects  in  muscle  and 
nerve  by  weak  stimuli  may  well  be  limitation  to  certain  of  the 
component  fibres,  whereas  a  maximal  stimulus  excites  them  all. 
It  is  a  matter  of  interest  how  far  this  numerical  factor  explains 
submaximal  responses  from  a  spinal  centre.  If  its  elements  are 
functionally  separate,  partial  responses  are  open  to  occur  in  such 
a  mechanism  since  it  is  multiple  as  regards  its  physiological 
components.  Gotch^**  has  recently  raised  this  question  in  an 
interesting  way.  He  points  out  that  in  the  electrical  organ  of 
Malapterurus,  where  the  whole  organ  is  innervated  by  a  single 
nerve-fibrey  the  reflex-response  is  little  variable  in  its  intensity 
in  comparison  with  the  wide  range  of  response  to  increasing 
stimulus-intensities  exhibited  by  stimulation  of  the  electric  nerve 
of  Torpedo,  —  a  structure  containing  many  nerve-fibres.  Con- 
cerning the  grading  of  motor  discharges  from  the  central  nervous 
system  he  asks,  —  **  Is  it  not   possible  that  these  grades  are 


t 


IIIJ 


GRADING   OF    INTENSITY   OF    REFLEX  75 


.2    o 


g  •£  -S  :S    i 


1 1ll-si 

«J       ^   •*     o     « 
rd    (4    V'    ^    en 

11-1   cit 


^  ¥ 


rs     3 


—    C 


«    «   o    o        o 
I    3  ^  "->  £    o 

,,  o  o  o  s 


E 


55    rt     O  t3    O    O 


§1 


S   V   >    Z   >  ^   a 

^!s8;|.l| 

c   "   «   !^:5  -   g 

iij-^  t  S  -  ^ 


■S   o 

5  j: 


bC  C 


^  3 


i«  i:  -  " 


•S  j=  5  s       « 


cali-S 


m    O 


J=  ^    S    «    C 

c 
o 


-S  J  «  §  -  -5  2 


1 1  ^  ^  £  i^  I 


76  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

largely  dependent  on  the  number  of  central  elements  involved, 
and  are  only  incidentally  associated  with  variations  in  the  in- 
tensity of  the  nervous  process  in  any  one  neurone?" 

That  in  spinal  reflexes  increase  of  the  intensity  of  the  excit- 
ing stimulus  causes  increase  in  the  number  of  motor  neurones 
excited  is  clearly  shown  by  the  wider  field  of  musculature  seen 
to  be  engaged  as  the  reflex  irradiates  under  intenser  stimulation. 
This  is  the  well-known  spread  which  Pfliiger^*  endeavoured  to 
formulate  rules  to  express.  Within  one  and  the  same  muscle- 
groups,  and  even  within  one  and  the  same  individual  muscle, 
grading  of  intensity  of  reflex  contraction  by  this  numerical  im- 
plication of  more  or  fewer  motor-cells  seems  not  only  possible 
but  probable.  It  is  perhaps  one  object  of  their  multiplicity. 
The  want  of  difference  between  the  latent  time  of  the  incre- 
mental and  initial  reflexes  mentioned  above  (Lect.  I,  p.  24) 
might  be  explicable  thus. 

With  very  feeble  stimuli,  or  under  spinal  shock,  when  only 
feeble  reflex  reactions  can  be  evoked,  it  is  easy  to  see />artial  con- 
tractions of  muscles,  e.  g.,  in  the  tibialis  anticus,  sartorius,  and 
semimembranosus.  Under  like  circumstances  the  scratch-reflex 
may  have  the  form  of  simply  a  feeble  rhythmic  dorsi-flexion  at 
ankle  and  toes,  or  even  of  the  toes  alone.  We  have  referred  to 
such  phenomena  before,  and  they  harmonize  well  with  the  view 
of  a  fractional  activity  of  the  motor  centre  of  the  scratch  and 
other  reflexes. 

Yet  we  must  not  lose  sight  of  the  physiological  solidarity  of 
the  action  of  the  group  of  elements  that  compose  a  '*  reflex 
centre "  in  its  reflex  activity.  "  Immediate  spinal  induction " 
{v,  infray  Lect.  IV)  and  the  spatial  spread  of  the  refractory 
phase  in  scratch-reflexes  evoked  from  separate  points  show 
(Lect.  II,  p.  60)  that,  intraspinally,  the  various  component  arcs 
of  the  type-reflex  are  interconnected  to  something  like  a  unitary 
mechanism.  Further,  the  evidence  that  when  the  scratch-reflex 
is  being  elicited  from  one  point  the  refractory  period  obtains 

Figure  27.  —  The  crossed  extension-reflex  showing  grades  of  intensity  corresponding  with 
grading  of  intensity  of  stimulus.  The  time  of  stimulation  is  shown  by  the  signal  on  the 
line  above,  and  in  each  case  consisted  of  12  break  shocks  delivered  at  the  rate  of  25  per 


GRADING   OF   INTENSITY   OF   REFLEX 


17 


\r\  t^  ^  \r\ 


<  m  cj  Q  w 


h  n 


S.S 

•^1 

.2  1^ 

•^ts 

*8   ^ 

Is 

tS  5 

78  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

practically  throughout  its  intraspinal  centre  indicates  the  same 
functional  unity.  The  nerve-cells  building  the  centre  seem 
combined  like  those  of  the  nerve-net  of  Medusa.  The  ele- 
ments s^em  incapable  of  isolated  excitation.  Such  an  intra- 
spinal group  as  that  involved  in  the  scratch-reflex  must  extend 
through  a  considerable  length  of  the  cord.  And  yet  though 
interconnected,  like  the  web  of  Medusa's  nerve-net,  there  is 
evidence  that  in  the  various  reflex  forms  which  the  scratching 
movement  takes  according  as  elicited  from  one  point  or  an- 
other, one  part  of  the  centre  is  the  more  active  in  one  form  of 
the  reflex  and  another  in  another  form  of  the  reflex.  The 
mechanism  is  not  therefore  always  equally  aff^ected  through- 
out, and  in  this  inequality  numerical  proportion  of  active  to 
inactive  elements  may  play  a  part.  The  mechanism,  neverthe- 
less, although  anatomically  an  assemblage  of  units,  is  function- 
ally itself  a  unit.  And  a  comparable  solidarity  obtains  in  other 
reflex  mechanisms.  Even  the  irradiation  which  suggests  exten- 
sion to  new  units  itself  gives  evidence  of  the  welding  of  the 
unit  elements  of  centres  together  into  functional  unit  groups 
possessing  solidarity.  When  in  the  flexion-reflex  the  response 
spreads  from  the  knee  to  the  hip,  the  spread  is  not  gradual, 
but  the  hip  flexion  suddenly  comes  in,  marking  a  sharp  step- 
like rise  on  the  record  (Fig.  45,  p.  153).  It  is  not  as  though  the 
irradiation  gradually  reached  the  motor  elements  of  the  hip- 
flexion  centre  cell  by  cell:  the  irradiation  on  involving  that 
centre  forthwith  evokes  discharge  from  it  which,  judging  from 
its  powerful  effect,  represents  discharge  from  the  centre  practi- 
cally as  a  whole.  The  reaction  as  it  irradiates  treats  the  centre 
as  a  unit. 

The  great  prolongation  of  the  reflex-discharge  produced  by 
intensifying,  apart  from  prolonging,  the  external  stimulus  is 
also  against  the  increase  of  discharge  being  explicable  merely 
or  chiefly  by  implication  of  a  greater  number  of  motor  ele- 
ments. In  the  flexion-reflex,  the  period  of  discharge  may  be 
lengthened  tenfold  by  increasing  simply  the  intensity  of  the 
stimulus  without  lengthening  it.  In  the  "  crossed-extension 
reflex  "  I  have  seen  the  period  of  discharge  lengthened  more 


I 


Ill]         OXYGEN   AND   REFLEX   CONDUCTION  79 

than  twenty-fold.  This  argues  that  the  grading  of  the  motor  dis- 
charge in  these  reflexes  is  in  important  measure  due  to  graded 
intensities  of  discharge  from  the  unit  elements  themselves,  of 
which  the  reflex  centres  are  compounded.  The  functional  unity 
of  a  reflex  centre  seems  also  evident  from  the  fact  that  it  is 
the  instrument  of  a  number  of  receptive  organs  scattered  over 
a  relatively  wide  field  —  a  field  which  for  the  flexion-reflex  is 
almost  coextensive  with  the  whole  skin  surface  of  the  limb  — 
and  nevertheless  an  intense  stimulus  from  any  limited  part  of 
that  field  can  elicit  a  reflex  of  full  strength.  This  it  can  only 
attain  if  the  whole  centre  of  the  reflex  be  at  its  disposal.  There- 
fore, as  we  saw  in  the  scratch-reflex,  the  whole  motor  centre 
potentially  belongs  to  all  and  each  of  the  groups  of  receptive 
organs  proper  to  the  reflex.  The  centre,  although  consisting  of 
anatomical  units  which  are  individual,  seems  knitted  together 
functionally.  It  is  not  necessarily  the  motor  cells  which  conjoin 
—  were  that  so  one  hardly  sees  how  stimulation  of  the  central 
end  of  a  motor  root  could  fail  to  excite  discharge  from  other 
motor  roots,  which  the  Bell-Magendie  law  shows  that  it  does 
actually  fail  to  do. 

Reflex  conduction  less  resistant  than  nerve  conduction.  The 
diff'erences  traced  thus  far  between  reflex-arc  conduction  and 
nerve-trunk  conduction  have  been  difl^erences  brought  out  by 
variations  of  stimulus  and  other  external  conditions.  Differ- 
ences no  less  notable  appear  under  changes  of  internal  kind. 
Without  entering  on  these  fully,  a  glance  at  them  is  helpful 
for  the  understanding  of  reflex-arc  conduction. 

Conduction  by  nerve-trunks  is  but  slowly  affected  by  inter- 
ference with  blood-supply;  reflexes  are  nevertheless  among  the 
earliest  reactions  to  alter  or  fail  under  asphyxial  conditions. 
V.  Baeyer  ^^  found  the  sciatic  nerve  of  the  frog  retain  excitabil- 
ity and  conductivity  three  to  five  hours  in  nitrogen,  and  on  read- 
mission  of  oxygen  regain  its  powers  in  a  few  minutes.  Bergmann 
(cited  by  Biedermann)  found  interruption  of  the  circulation  in  the 
frog  extinguish  reflexes  in  thirty  minutes.  Verworn  has  shown 
that  the  spinal  centres  of  a  strychnized  frog,  if  unsupplied  with 
oxygen,  fail  to  react  in  about  an  hour's  time,  but  are  promptly 


8o  THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect.  ] 

restored  by  resupply  of  oxygen.  Baglioni  ^^  has  shown  that  1 
the  spinal  centres  of  the  frog,  deprived  of  circulation  and  im- 1 
mersed  in  nitrogen,  fail  to  give  reflexes  in  about  forty-five  j 
minutes,  but  in  an  atmosphere  of  oxygen  continue  to  react  j 
for  twenty  hours.  | 

Again,  the  dosage  of  chloroform  or  ether  required  to  depress  \ 
and  abolish  nerve-trunk  conduction   is  much  greater   than  is  ( 
required  to  depress  and  abolish  the  cerebro-spinal  reflexes.     In  \ 
Waller's  observations  on  extinction  of  action-current  in  nerve-  j 
trunks  a  3  per  cent  dose  of  chloroform  in  air  was    required,  l 
Using  in  the  cat  indirect  contraction  of  the  gastrocnemius  as 
an  index  of  sciatic  nerve-conduction,  Miss  Sowton  and  myself 
found   .3   per  cent   chloroform  in  diluted  blood  at  36°  C.  re- 
quired   to    abolish   the   reaction    (Fig.    28).     This    is    a    much  <\ 
higher  percentage  than  suffices  to  depress  the  heart's  action.  ■ 
Since   many  reflexes   are   abolished   by   doses   which   do   not  i 
markedly  depress  the  heart,  reflex  conduction  is  abolished  by  \ 
doses  a  fortiori  smaller  than  those  which  set  aside  nerve-trunk  fi 
conduction. 

Again,  a  number  of  agents,  e.  g.y  strychnine,  tetanus  toxin,  ; 
etc.,  that  do  not  appreciably  affect  nerve-trunk  conduction  \ 
enormously  alter  reflex-arc  conduction.  All  these  seem  to  "\ 
exert  their  influence  on  some  part  of  the  reflex  conductor  which  \ 
lies  in  gray  matter.     It  is  interesting  to  ask  whether  they,  e.  g.  \ 

Figure  28  (opposite).  —  A.  Hind  limb  of  cat.    Perfused  with  dilute  blood.  Contractions  of  gas-  ^ 

trocnemius  muscle  stimulated  through  its  nerve.    Effect  of  CHClj  at  0.25  per  cent.    The  \ 

register  of  flow  of  the  blood  through  the  blood-vessels  shows  a  diminution  at  first,  and  then  ■ 

a  marked  increase.     The  register  of  flow  is  the  bottom  line :  each  notch  in  that  line  in-  ; 

dicates  one  emptying  of  the  Schafer  "  tilter  "  receiving  the  blood  at  outflow  from  the  limb.  J 

The  line  next  above  marks  the  time  in  intervals  of  fifteen  seconds.     The  third  line  from  \ 

bottom  signals  the  perfusion  of  blood  containing  chloroform  0.25  per  cent;  similar  blood,  \ 

but  free  from  chloroform,  being  perfused  before  and  after.     The  top  line  indicates  the  : 

pressure  of  delivery  of  the  perfused  blood  at  the  entrant  cannula.     The  chloroform  reduces  ^ 

the  contractions  of  the  muscle,  stimulated  through  its  nerve,  by  more  than  a  half.  3 

B.   Same  as  above.  Contractions  of  gastrocnemius  muscle  stimulated  alternately,  through  1 

its  nerve,  and  directly.     The  nerve  was  inexcitable  before  the  perfusing  fluid  was  turned  on,  •; 

and  the  record  which  begins  immediately  after  perfusion  had  been  started  shows  the  gradual  ] 

recovery  of  excitability.     CHCl,  at  0.3  per  cent  abolishes  the  response  of  the  muscle  to  2 

indirect  stimulation,  and  reduces  its  direct  response  almost  to  zero.     The  second,  fourth,  ; 

sixth,  etc.,  are  direct  responses  of  the  muscle;  the  first,  third,  fifth,  etc.,  are  responses  to  \ 

stimulation  through  the  nerve-trunk.     In  this  tracing  the  time  record  is  above  the  signal  ■ 
record.     (Sowton  and  Sherrington.) 


"n 


I]     CHLOROFORM  AND  REFLEX  CONDUCTION     8i 


r 
§ 

o 


/    _ 


82  THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

strychnine,  have  an  effect  similar  to  their  spinal  effect  when 
exhibited  in  Bethe's  preparation  of  the  second  antenna  ganglion 
of  Carcinus,  whence  the  motor  perikarya  have  been  removed. 
If  these  agents  have  their  locus  of  incidence  at  the  synapse,  it 
must  be  conceded  that  they  act  with  very  different  intensities 
at  different  synapses. 

From  this  rehearsal  of  the  differences  between  nerve-trunk 
conduction  and  reflex-arc  conduction  it  seems  evident  that  cer- 
tain elements  of  co-ordination  of  "  the  simple  reflex  "  are  to  be 
found  in  the  qualities  of  conduction  of  the  reflex-arc.  Each  of 
the  various  types  of  simple  reflex  possesses  to  a  large  extent  its 
own  peculiarities  of  conduction.  Though  there  are  differences 
between  conduction  in  various  nerve-trunks,  e.  g.y  in  speed  of 
transmission  of  impulses,  etc.,  these  differences  sink  to  insignifi- 
cance when  contrasted  with  the  extent  and  variety  of  the  con- 
ductive differences  exhibited  by  different  reflex-arcs.  And  in  the 
case  of  each  reflex-arc  its  idiosyncrasies  of  conduction  form  an 
obvious  basis  for  the  co-ordination  exhibited  by  its  reflex-act 

Functions  of  the  perikaryon.  —  It  may  appear  that  our  tend- 
ency is  to  attribute  the  distinctive  characters  of  reflex-arc 
conduction  so  liberally  to  the  synapse  that  the  perikaryon 
is  stripped  of  all  functions  and  only  equivalent  to  a  piece  of 
nerve-fibre.  But  it  is  to  be  remembered  that  two  functions 
of  great  importance  certainly  belong  to  the  perikaryon.  In 
the  first  place  it,  even  if  the  conductive  process  in  it  be  wholly 
similar  to  that  of  a  nerve-fibre,  is  at  least  a  place  where  the  con- 
ductor branches,  often  to  such  an  extent  as  occurs  nowhere  else, 
so  that  it  is  a  nodal  point  in  the  spatial  distribution  of  the  con- 
ductive lines.  In  the  second  place,  there  seems  no  valid  reason 
yet  to  doubt  the  long-held  view  that  regards  the  perikaryon  as 
the  nutritive  centre  of  the  neurone  to  which  it  belongs. 

Certain  features  mentioned  already  as  saliently  distinguish- 
ing reflex-conduction  from  nerve-trunk  conduction  still  remain 
for  consideration.  Among  these  are  fatigability,  facilitation, 
inhibitory  interference,  spinal  induction.  These  will,  however, 
be  better  taken  under  the  compounding  of  reflexes.    One  feature 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL   INHIBITION  83 

that  we  have  not  considered  may,  however,  with  advantage  be 
considered  at  once.     This  feature  is  inhibition. 

Reciprocal  inhibition.  —  In  the  end-effect  of  certain  reflexes, 
for  instance  the  scratch-reflex,  there  supervenes  on  a  phase  of  ex- 
citatory state  a  state  refractory  to  excitation  —  a  refractory  phase. 
This  refractory  phase  is,  if  we  seek  to  put  it  into  the  class  of 
physiological  phenomena  to  which  it  must  obviously  belong,  a 
state  of  inhibition.  In  the  scratch-reflex  we  have  therefore  a 
reflex  in  which  an  external  stimulus  evokes  as  its  end-effect  an 
excitatory  phase,  succeeded  by  an  inhibitory  phase,  and  this  suc- 
cession in  this  reflex,  the  stimuli  being  continued,  is  repeated 
many  times.  If  we  denote  excitation  as  an  end-effect  by  the 
sign  plus  (-I-),  and  inhibition  as  end-effect  by  the  sign  minus  (— ), 
such  a  reflex  as  the  scratch-reflex  can  be  termed  a  reflex  of 
double-sign,  for  it  develops  excitatory  end-effect  and  then  in- 
hibitory end-effect  even  during  the  duration  of  the  exciting 
stimulus. 

There  is  a  further  numerous  class  of  reflexes  in  which 
the  end-effect  consists  both  in  excitatory  state  and  in  in- 
hibitory state,  but  the  inhibitory  state  does  not  supervene 
on  the  excitatory  or  have  the  same  locus  of  incidence  as  the 
excitatory;  it  occurs  simultaneously  with  it  at  another  inter- 
related locus.  The  ordinary  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  of  the 
spinal  cat  and  dog  is  a  reflex  of  this  type.  The  end-effect  of 
the  reflex  is  expressed  by  two  groups  of  muscles  whose  con- 
tractions act  in  opposed  direction  at  the  same  joints.  This 
opposition  is  obviated  in  the  end-effect  of  the  reflex  by  the  end- 
effect  having  the  form  of  excitatory  state  as  regards  the  motor- 
nerve  to  the  flexor  muscle,  but  suppression  or  withholding  of 
excitatory  state  (central  inhibition)  as  regards  the  motor  neurone 
of  the  extensor.  Such  reflex  is  a  reflex  of  double-sign,  but 
whereas  the  scratch-reflex  and  the  eyelid-reflex,  etc.,  are  re- 
flexes with  successive  double-sign,  the  flexion-reflex  and  reflexes 
of  that  type,  e.  g.,  the  crossed  extension-reflex,  are  reflexes  with 
simultaneous  double-sign. 

The  form  in  which  this  central  inhibition  occurs  may  be  best 
gathered  from  illustrative  examples. 


1 


84  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

'I 

The  simple  reflex  mechanism  examined  in  the  swimming-  | 

bell  of  Medusa  gives  little   evidence  of  an  arrangement  for  a  ; 

form  of  spatial  co-ordination  which  is  very  prevalent  in  more  | 

complex  mechanisms.     In  many  cases  the  body,  or  some  part  i 

of  it,  can  be  actively  moved,  not  merely  in  one  direction  but  j 

in  two  or  more,  opposed  or  partially  opposed.     The  muscula-  ^ 

ture  is  then  usually  divided  into  various  discrete  pieces  called  1 

**  muscles."    The  contraction  of  one  muscle,  or  set  of  muscles,  ;] 

produces  movement  in  one  direction;    the  contraction  of  an-  • 

other  produces  movement  in  another  direction.     Instances  of  '; 

this  are  common  in  the  limbs,  neck,  tail,  etc.,  of  Vertebrates  ; 

and  Arthropods.  l 

Reflex  co-ordination  makes  separate  muscles  whose  contrac-  : 

tions  act  harmoniously,  e.  g.  on  a  lever,  contract  together,  although  • 

at  separate  places,  so  that  they  assist  toward  the  same  end.     In  ; 

other  words,  it  excites  synergic  muscles.     But  it  in  many  cases  ; 

does  more  than  that.     Where  two  muscles  would  antagonize  ; 
each  other's  action  the  reflex-arc,  instead  of  activating  merely 

one  of  the  two,  causes  when  it  activates  the  one  depression  of  ^ 

the  activity  (tonic  or  rhythmic  contraction)  of  the  other.     The  j 
latter  is  an  inhibitory  effect. 

Classical  examples  of  inhibition  are  those  of  the  vagus  nerve  [ 

on  the  heart,  and  of  the  corda  tympani  on  the  blood-vessels  of  '\ 

the  submaxillary  region.     In  these  cases  the  stimulation  of  the  \ 

distal  end  of  a  peripheral  nerve  quells  the  existing  contraction  ■ 

of  the    muscles   of  the    heart   and    blood-vessel    respectively.  \ 

When  the  submaxillary  gland    is  called   into  activity  reflexly,  \ 

depression  of  the  tonic   contraction  of  the  muscular  coat  of  j 

its  arteries  accompanies  the  heightened   secretory  activity  of  \ 

gland  cells  simultaneously  evoked.     The  two  reflex  actions — the  \ 

one  depressing  the  activity  of  one  tissue,  the  other  heightening  \ 

that  of  the  other  tissue  —  are  mutually  co-operative,  and  are  ^ 

combined  in  the  one  reflex  action,  and  are  instances  of  a  reflex  • 

co-ordination  quite  comparable  with  that  in  which  one  muscle  \ 

of  an  antagonistic  couple  is  thrown  out  of  action  when  the  other  \ 

is  brought  into  action.     And  as  in  this  case,  so  in  some  cases  \ 

of  mutual  co-operation  of  inhibition  with  pressor  action  in  the  \ 


Ill]  INHIBITION   IN    REFLEXES  85 

nervous  regulation  of  antagonistic  muscles,  the  inhibition  is 
peripheral;  that  is  to  say,  stimulation  of  the  distal  piece  of  the 
divided  peripheral  nerve  itself  suffices  to  produce  it.  Instances 
of  this  occur  in  the  claw  of  Astacus,  and  in  the  muscles  opening 
the  shell  of  the  bivalve  Anodon.  In  Astacus,  as  is  well  known, 
(Richet,*^  Biedermann,^^  Piotrowski  ^*^)  stimulation  of  the  distal 
end  of  the  cut  peripheral  nerve  causes,  under  suitable  condi- 
tions, relaxation  of  the  closing  muscle  at  the  same  time  as 
contraction  of  the  opening  muscle.  This  is  comparable  with 
the  stimulation  of  the  distal  end  of  the  cut  corda  tympanic  which 
produces  relaxation  of  the  muscular  coat  of  the  arteries  of  the 
submaxillary  gland  at  the  same  time  as  it  causes  secretion  by 
the  gland  cells. 

The  muscles  of  the  claw  of  Astacus  are  striate,  and  the  case 
is  interesting  as  one  in  which  the  co-ordination  of  action  of  two 
antagonistic  muscles  of  skeletal  type  is  effected  by  peripheral 
inhibition  of  one  through  the  same  nerve-trunk  that  induces 
active  contraction  of  the  other.  But  the  similar  co-ordination 
in  the  taxis  of  the  skeletal  musculature  of  vertebrates  exerts 
its  inhibition  not  at  the  periphery  but  in  the  nerve-centres.  It 
occurs  within  the  gray  matter  of  the  central  nervous  system. 

When  the  spinal  cord  has  been  transected  headward  of  the 
lumbar  region,  reflex  movements  of  the  hind  limb  can,  after 
the  period  of  shock  has  passed,  be  studied  with  much  uniformity 
of  result.  Electric  stimuli  apphed  to  the  skin  of  the  limb, 
especially  of  the  foot,  evoke  practically  uniformly  a  drawing  up 
of  the  limb.  This  flexion-reflex,  as  presented  by  the  spinal 
dog,  consists  in  flexion  at  knee,  hip,  and  ankle. 

The  afferent  fibres  from  each  even  small  area  of  the  skin  of 
the  foot  do  not  enter  together  as  a  tiny  group  into  the  spinal 
cord  in  any  single  filament  of  a  single  afferent  root,  but  scatter 
and  make  their  entrance  into  the  cord  via  a.  number  of  root- 
lets,^^^  belonging  not  merely  to  one  but  to  two  or  even  three 
adjacent  afferent  spinal  roots.  These  afferent  fibres  having 
entered  the  cord,  severally  subdivide  in  the  manner  well  known 
since  the  researches  of  Nansen,  Ramon,  Van  Gehuchten,  v. 
Lenhossek,  and  others ;  and  their  collaterals  and  terminals  must, 


86  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

as  it  were,  seek  out  the  motor  cells  of  the  above-cited  flexor 
muscles,  and,  as  it  might  appear  from  the  above  evidence,  leave 
the  motor  cells  of  other  muscles,  for  instance,  of  the  extensors, 
alone.  Increase  of  intensity  of  the  stimulation  of  the  plantar 
skin  does  not  in  my  experience  make  the  spinal  reflex  action 
flow  over,  so  to  say,  from  the  flexor  muscles  to  the  extensors. 
As  the  strength  of  the  stimulus  is  increased  from  minimal,  the 
number  of  the  flexor  muscles  obviously  thrown  into  action  in 
the  limb  is  increased,  and  the  reaction  irradiates  to  other  regions 
of  the  body ;  for  instance,  to  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  contra- 
lateral hind  leg.  In  the  muscles  already  implicated  in  the 
weaker  response  the  contraction  becomes,  as  the  stimulus  is  in- 
creased, stronger,  but  I  have  not  found  it  involve  the  muscles, 
causing  extension  of  the  homolateral  hind  limb  itself.  This 
flexor-reflex  of  the  limb  therefore  appears,  although  able  to 
excite  to  various  degrees  of  activity  the  flexor  musculature  of 
the  limb,  unable  to  excite  the  extensor  musculature. 

It  would  be  a  mistake,  however,  to  suppose  that  it  is  without 
any  direct  influence  on  the  latter  musculature.  It  might  appear 
from  the  statement  that  the  distribution  of  the  aflerent  con- 
ductors of  the  reflex  was  to  the  motor  neurones  of  flexion  only, 
and  not  to  those  of  the  extensor  muscles.  But  the  motor 
neurones  of  the  extensor  muscles  are  not  inaccessible  to  im- 
pulses arriving  by  this  afferent  path.  On  the  contrary,  they  can 
be  shown  to  be  easily  and  habitually  accessible  to  them. 

To  examine  this  we  may  turn  to  the  "  knee-jerk,"  and  to 
the  tonus  of  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  knee.  In  the  spinal 
animal,  for  instance  in  the  dog  and  cat,  after  transection  of  the 
spinal  cord  in  the  thoracic  region,  it  is  easy  to  satisfy  one's  self 
that,  after  the  shock  has  passed  off"  the  extensor  muscles  of  the 
knee  still  possess  considerable  tonus.  The  spinal  tonus  is  reflex, 
and  it  has  been  shown  ^*  that  in  the  crureus  and  vastus  medialis 
muscles  of  the  cat,  the  reflex  tonus  of  those  muscles  is  traceable 
to  aff"erent  nerves  arising  in  those  very  muscles  themselves. 

The  reflex-arc  through  which  the  tonus  is  produced  and 
maintained  arises  in  those  muscles  themselves  and  returns  to 
them  again.     The  knee-jerk  is  easily  elicited  in  the  spinal  cat 

■A 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL   INHIBITION  87 

and  dog.  The  muscles  which  contract  when  the  patellar  tendon 
is  struck  are  in  these  animals  the  vastus  medialis  and  crtirens}^ 
The  knee-jerk  seems,  however,  only  obtainable  in  them  when 
their  reflex  spinal  tonus  is  present.  Its  briskness  varies  pari 
passu  with  the  degree  of  this  tonus.  Severance  of  the  affer- 
ent nerves  of  these  muscles  destroys  their  tonus,  and  renders  at 
the  same  time  the  knee-jerk  inelicitable,  just  as  also  does  the 
severance  of  their  motor-nerves. 

The  knee-jerk  is,  therefore,  Hke  the  spinal  tonus  itself,  de- 
pendent on  the  integrity  of  the  reflex  spinal  arc  of  the  muscles. 
But  it  is  customary  to  regard  the  knee-jerk  not  as  a  reflex  action 
(Westphal,  Waller,  and  others)  ;  hence  it  is  termed  "  knee- 
phenomenon,"  "  knee-jerk,"  etc.  The  main  ground  for  denying 
its  claim  to  be  really  reflex  is  that  its  latent  period  is  shorter 
than  that  of  other  indubitable  reflexes.  The  latency  for  the 
knee-jerk  has  been  shown  (Waller,^^^  Gotch,^^"^  and  others)  to 
be  about  10  <r,  whereas  the  shortest  latency  found  by  Exner^  for 
reflex  eyelid-closure  was  45  o-  and  by  Fr.  Franck  ^"  for  a  spinal 
reflex  about  i/o".  The  latency  for  the  knee-jerk  is  but  little 
longer  than  that  for  direct  excitation  of  the  extensor  muscle 
itself. 

If  we  regard  the  knee-jerk  not  as  a  true  reflex  but  as  a 
"direct"  response  of  the  muscle,  we  have  to  suppose  that  the 
reflex  tonus  of  the  muscle,  which  is  admittedly  a  conditio  siiie 
qua  non  for  the  jerk,  so  raises  the  direct  excitability  of  the 
muscle  that  the  muscle  responds  by  a  contraction  to  a  sudden 
slight  stretch  of  itself  due  to  a  tap  on  its  tendon.  No  experi- 
menter has,  however,  satisfactorily  succeeded  by  artificial  stimula- 
tion of  the  motor-nerve  in  similarly  raising  the  direct  excitability 
of  the  muscle.  Moreover,  Gotch^^^  found  the  muscle  in  its 
state  of  tonus  gave  no  other  indication  of  increased  excitability 
than  simply  that  it  then  yielded  **  the  jerk."  It  has  been  urged 
against  the  reflex  nature  of  the  jerk  that  the  contraction  given 
by  the  muscle  to  the  jerk  is  a  simple  twitch.  The  "  jerk  "  con- 
traction lasts  no  longer,  or  hardly  longer,  than  the  twitch  given 
by  the  muscle  in  response  to  a  single  stimulus,  e.g.^  an  induction 
shock.     All  reflex  contractions  are  usually  considered  as  tetanic. 


1 

88  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

That  is  in  the  main  doubtless  true.  It  is  what  might  be  inferred 
from  the  great  part  played  by  summation  of  stimuli  in  the  elici- 
tation  of  reflexes.  Yet  the  extensor-thrust  reflex,  which  is 
undoubtedly  a  true  reflex,  appears  on  measurement  (p.  ^j)  to 
be  as  brief  as  the  knee-jerk.  Its  time-relations  have  been  re- 
ferred to.  It  is  interesting  that  this  brief-lasting  reflex  also  has, 
as  has  the  knee-jerk  itself,  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  hind 
limb  for  its  seat  of  expression.  The  mere  brevity  of  the  period 
of  contraction  of  the  knee-jerk  is  therefore  no  good  evidence 
that  it  is  not  reflex. 

The  knee-jerk,  whether  reflex  or  not,  furnishes,  since  it 
is  an  index  of  the  reflex  tonus  of  the  extensor  muscles,  a 
gauge  for  the  effect,  if  any,  exerted  by  the  flexion-reflex 
on  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  limb.  It  was  said  above 
that  the  extensors  are  not  thrown  into  contraction  by  this 
flexion-reflex.  The  reflex  reaction  may  therefore  either  be 
neutral  to  them  and  leave  them  and  their  condition  untouched, 
or  it  may  inhibit  them  and  depress  their  reflex  activity,  even  if 
that  activity  have  at  the  time  only  the  form  of  tonus. 

If  the  hamstring  muscles  (flexors  of  the  knee)  be  separated 
from  their  attachments  at  their  distal  (knee)  end,  and  then  while 
the  knee  joint  is  passively  held  in  approximate  or  full  extension 
the  flexor-reflex  be  elicited,  e.  g.  by  electric  stimulation  of  the 
foot,  the  extensor  muscles  above  the  knee  are  easily  felt  by  pal- 
pation to  at  once  lose  their  tonus  and  relax.^^  At  the  same 
moment  the  exposed  and  freed  flexor  muscles  are  seen  to  enter 
contraction.  That  is  to  say,  the  same  exciting  stimulus  that 
reflexly  throws  the  flexors  into  contraction  interrupts  reflexly 
the  reflex  tonus  of  the  extensor  muscles.  If  the  knee-jerk  be 
elicited  at  regular  short  intervals,  signalled  for  instance  by  a 
metronome,  and  while  it  is  in  progress  the  flexor-reflex  be  elic- 
ited after  the  flexor  muscles  have  been  detached  from  their 
knee  attachments  and  the  knee  thus  left  free,  the  knee-jerk 
is  found  inelicitable  or  much  diminished  directly  the  reflex 
contraction  of  the  hamstring  muscles  sets  in  (Fig.  29).  This 
inhibition  of  the  jerk  sometimes  seems  to  set  in  even  before 
the  reflex  contraction  of  the   flexors   is  apparent.     It  occurs 


Ill]  INHIBITION   OF   KNEE-JERK  89 

sometimes  when  the  stimukis  is  not  even  strong  enough  to  evoke 
obvious  contraction  of  the  flexors.  In  the  "  flexion-reflex," 
therefore,  the  reflex  excitation  of  the  flexor  muscles  is  accom- 
panied by  reflex  inhibition  of  the  antagonistic  extensor  muscles 
both  as  regards  their  reflex  tonus  which  is  in  progress  when  the 
flexor-reflex  is  excited,  and  as  regards  their  response  to  a  stim- 
ulus (tap  on  tendon  or  muscle)  that  otherwise  excites  them. 


Figure  29.  —  Tracing  from  preparation  of  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  knee,  recording  a 
series  of  knee-jerks  elicited  at  each  alternate  beat  of  a  metronome.  Weak  faradization  of 
the  central  end  of  the  hamstring  nerve  was  applied  during  the  time  marked  by  the 
signal  line  below.  The  tonus  of  the  extensor  preparation  at  once  fell,  and  with  it  the 
knee-jerk  was  temporarily  abolished.  After  cessation  of  the  inhibiting  stimulus  the  tonus 
and  the  knee-jerk  quickly  returned,  and  the  latter  became  more  brisk  than  previous  to  the 
inhibition.     The  lowest  line  marks  the  time  in  seconds. 

A  corresponding  reaction  is  seen  also  after  ablation  of  the 
cerebral  hemispheres  and  thalamencephalon.  After  removal 
of  those  organs,  there  ensues  "  decerebrate  rigidity."  *^»  ^^  One 
feature  of  this  condition  is  a  heightened  tonus  of  the  extensor 
muscles  of  the  knee.  The  knee  is  maintained  rigidly  extended. 
At  the  same  time  the  knee-jerk  is  elicitable  in  unusual  degree. 
When  the  knee  is  under  these  circumstances  freed  from  the 
flexor  muscles,  and  the  flexor-reflex  is  then  induced  by  appro- 
priate excitation,  e.g.y  of  the  plantar  skin,  the  knee  joint  at  once 
drops  loose,  and  if  the  knee-jerk  be  tested,  it  is  found  to  be 
inelicitable,  or  elicitable  only  very  faintly  (Fig.  29). 

Similarly,  if  instead  of  the  knee-jerk  or  the  reflex  rigidity 
of  the  decerebrate  animal,  we  take  the  reflex  termed  the  ex- 


90  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

tensor-thrust  as  a  guide  to  the  condition  of  the  extensor  arcs 
during  the  flexion-reflex,  we  get  similar  evidence  that  those  arcs 
are  temporarily  out  of  action.  While  the  flexion-reflex  is  in 
progress  the  extensor-thrust  is  less  elicitable.  If  the  flexion- 
reflex  is  quite  weak,  the  extensor-thrust  can  be  obtained  and 
breaks  through  it ;  but  it  cannot  if  the  flexion-reflex  be  of  fair 
or  of  considerable  intensity.  The  reflex  called  the  extensor- 
thrust  is  an  extremely  powerful  one ;  it  can  in  the  spinal  dog 
lift  the  whole  body  from  the  ground  and  push  it  forward. 
Yet  none  of  the  devices  normally  evoking  it  can  elicit  it  during 
a  fair  flexion-reflex.  It  becomes  elicitable  again  when  the 
flexion-reflex  is  over. 

It  seems,  therefore,  that  in  the  flexion-reflex  and  in  the  other 
above-mentioned  reflexes  an  inhibitory  process  is  part  and 
parcel  of  the  reflex  reaction,  so  that  the  inhibition  goes  on  side 
by  side  with  excitation  of  other  muscles  opposed  to  those  which 
are  inhibited.  This  view,  that  the  inhibition  process  in  these 
reflexes  is  a  simultaneous  counterpart  to  the  excitatory,  is 
supported  by  the  following  evidence  from  the  flexion-reflex. 

A  salient  feature  of  this  reflex  is  flexion  at  the  knee.  For 
comparison  of  the  inhibition  and  excitation  respectively,  both 
hind  limbs  are  taken  and  so  prepared  that  in  one  leg  only  the 
knee  flexors  can  act,  in  the  other  leg  only  the  knee-extensors. 
The  stimuli  to  provoke  the  reflex  are  applied  either  to  sym- 
metrical skin  points  or  to  symmetrical  afferent  nerves  at,  as  far 
as  practicable,  symmetrical  places  in  their  course.  For  compari- 
son, the  stimuli  are  made  as  far  as  possible  equal  on  the  two 
sides.  This  being  arranged,  certain  characteristic  features  of  the 
reflex  have  been  examined  on  the  two  sides  respectively. 

(a)  The  flexion-reflex  has  a  "  receptive  skin-field "  which 
though  extensive  is  characteristic  for  it.  Examined  by  the 
above  preparation  the  skin-field  whence  the  excitation  (con- 
traction) is  elicitable  and  that  whence  the  inhibition  is  elicitable 
has  proved  in  my  observations  to  be  one  and  the  same.  Thus : 
stigmatic  unipolar  faradization  of  a  point  in  the  skin  of  a  right 
pedal  digit  provokes  in  the  homonymous  limb  contraction  of  the 
flexors  of  the  knee,  and  similar  stimulation  of  the  correspond- 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL   INHIBITION  91 

ing  left  digit  provokes  in  its  own  limb  inhibition  of  the  extensors 
of  the  knee.  Again,  similar  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  fore 
foot  (in  my  experience  that  of  the  crossed  fore  foot  acts  more 
readily  than  that  of  the  homonymous)  induces  excitation  (con- 
traction) of  the  flexors  of  the  crossed  knee ;  and  the  correspond- 
ing skin-region  of  the  opposite  fore  limb  induces  inhibition 
(relaxation)  of  the  extensors  of  the  knee  contralateral  to  it. 

(yS)  Turning  to  stimuli  other  than  electrical,  it  is  not,  as  I 
have  pointed  out,  every  form  of  stimulus  that,  when  applied 
within  the  skin-field  appropriate  for  the  direct  flexion-reflex,  can 
excite  it.  The  kinds  of  skin-stimuli  which  excite  it  are  those 
which  may  be  termed  "  nocuous,"  ^^^  e.g'.,a  prick,  strong  squeeze, 
harmful  heat  (the  heat-beam),  and  chemical  agents.  Touches, 
innocuous  pressures,  rubbing,  etc.,  though  effective  for  various 
reflexes,  e.  g.y  for  the  extensor-thrust,  scratch-reflex,  pinna- 
reflex,  etc.,  do  not  in  my  experience  excite  this  reflex.  The 
stimuli  which  do  excite  it,  for  instance,  from  the  planta,  excite, 
when  applied  on  the  side  where  the  flexor  muscles  alone  remain 
intact,  contraction  of  those  muscles,  and  when  applied  corre- 
spondingly on  the  opposite  side,  where  the  extensors  alone 
remain  intact,  inhibit  them  (relaxation). 

(7)  The  nerve-twig,  similar  to  that  which  under  faradization 
on  the  **  flexors  "  side  excites  the  flexors  (contraction)  when 
faradized  on  the  "extensors"  side  inhibits  the  extensors  (re- 
laxation). This  comparison  has  been  made  not  only  with 
skin  nerves,  but  with  muscular  nerves,  notably  with  the  nerves 
of  the  hamstring  muscles  and  of  the  gastrocnemius. 

(S)  The  flexion-reflex,  although  it  exhibits  well  the  potency 
of  summation  of  successive  stimuli  as  a  factor  in  its  initia- 
tion, differs  in  my  experience  from  various  other  reflexes, 
e.  g.y  extensor-thrust,  scratch-reflex,  pinna-reflex,  in  being  elicit- 
able  fairly  easily  by  a  single-induction  shock.  The  shock 
may  be  applied  either  to  the  skin  in  the  receptive  skin- 
field  of  the  reflex  or  to  an  appropriate  afferent  nerve  either 
cutaneous  or  muscular.  When  this  is  done  in  the  prepared 
limbs  the  single-induction  shock  applied  on  the  **  flexors  "  side 
excites  a  brief  reflex  contraction  of  those  muscles,  correspond- 


92  THE   SIMPLE    REFLEX  [Lect. 

ingly  applied  on  the  "  extensors  "  side  it  provokes  a  brief  reflex 
inhibition  of  those  muscles. 

(e)  The  flexion-reflex,  unlike  extensor-thrust,  pinna-reflex, 
etc.,  can  be  well  evoked  in  my  experience  by  make  or  break 
of  a  galvanic  current.  This  make  or  break  reflex  is  shown 
in  the  "  extensor  "  preparation  by  inhibition,  just  as  it  is  shown 
in  the  "flexor"  preparation  by  contraction.  With  suitable 
strength  of  stimulus  the  break  of  a  descending  current  is  more 
effective  for  the  reflex  inhibition  than  the  make,  and  vice  versa 
for  an  ascending  current,  just  as  with  contraction.  The  flexion- 
reflex  can  also  to  a  much  greater  extent  than  can  the  scratch- 
reflex  be  maintained  by  passage  of  the  constant  current  In 
this  respect  it  resembles  the  vasomotor  and  respiratory  reflexes 
examined  by  Grutzner,'^  and  by  Langendorff  and  Oldag,^*^ 
and  also  the  sensual  reaction  which  similar  stimulation  excites 
in  ourselves  —  a  point  of  interest  when  the  connection  between 
noci'Ceptive  reflexes  and  dolorous  sensation  is  remembered. 
When  the  constant  current  is  thus  applied  to  the  limb  in  which 
the  extensors  have  been  prepared,  inhibition  proceeds  in  them 
as  does  contraction  in  the  flexors  when  that  current  is  similarly 
applied  to  the  limb  in  which  the  flexors  have  been  prepared. 

(JO  The  latent  time  of  the  flexion-reflex  is  short.  This 
feature  is  revealed  in  the  inhibition  of  the  extensors  just  as  in 
the  contraction  of  the  flexors.  Great  differences  of  latency 
in  the  flexion-reflex  as  in  other  reflexes  can  be  obtained  by, 
apart  from  variance  in  intrinsic  condition  of  the  reflex  prepara- 
tion, variance  in  the  external  stimuli  in  intensity,  suddenness, 
frequency  of  repetition,  etc.  The  effect  of  such  variations  is  the 
same  in  kind,  and,  in  my  experience,  in  extent,  when  tested  by 
the  reflex  inhibition  as  when  tested  by  the  reflex  contraction. 
Thus,  with  strong  stimuli  I  have  found  as  short  a  latency  as 
32  o-  for  the  inhibition,  which  is  slightly  shorter  than  the  shortest 
for  contraction  under  like  circumstances  that  I  have  yet  met 
with.  With  weak  stimuli  I  have  occasionally  met  with  a  latency 
as  long  as  400  a  for  each  effect. 

(r/)  A  good  criterion  of  comparison  between  the  reflex  in- 
hibition and  the  reflex  contraction  in  the  flexion-reflex  under 


I  ^ 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL  INHIBITION  93 

excitation  by  an  intermittent  stimulus  is  the  number  of  stimuli 
summed  for  initiation  of  the  reflex  as  exhibited  on  the  one  hand 
in  contraction  of  the  flexors,  on  the  other  hand  in  relaxation  of 
the  extensors.  The  number  of  successive  single  stimuli  summed 
for  the  initiation  is  less  as  their  individual  intensity  is  greater.^^ 

When  the  summation  is  compared  in  the  same  reflex  prepa- 
ration, in  the  reflex  exhibited  as  inhibition  (relaxation)  in  the 
knee-extensors  of  one  limb  and  in  the  reflex  exhibited  as  con- 
traction in  the  knee-flexors  of  the  other  limb,  good  agreement 
is  found ;  the  number  has  been  often  actually  the  same,  though 
the  observations  are  made  alternately,  first  one  on  one  limb,  then 
one  on  the  other  limb.  Figs.  30  A  and  30  B,  and  31  A  and  31  B 
are  such  pairs,  and  illustrate  the  kind  of  agreement. 

(^)  The  course  of  the  flexion-reflex  as  shown  in  myo- 
grams differs  much  from  that  of  certain  other  reflexes  of  the 
limb,  notably  from  the  extensor-thrust  and  from  the  scratch- 
reflex.  Its  duration  follows  more  closely  that  of  the  eliciting 
stimulus.  If  the  stimulus  is  quite  brief  and  not  intense  the 
myogram  shows  but  a  short  continuance  of  the  development 
of  the  effect  after  the  external  stimulus  itself  has  ceased.  The 
flexion-reflex  by  adjustment  of  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus 
can  be  graded  as  to  its  amplitude.  This  grading  is  seen  not 
only  as  a  grading  of  the  amplitude  of  contraction  of  the  flexors 
when  the  stimulus  is  applied  to  the  limb  with  intact  knee- 
flexors,  but  as  a  grading  of  the  amplitude  of  relaxation  when 
the  stimulus  is  applied  to  the  limb  with  intact  knee-extensors. 

These  correspondences  support  the  view  that  the  reflex 
inhibition  (relaxation)  and. the  reflex  excitation  (contraction) 
are  part  and  parcel  of  one  and  the  same  reflex  reaction  ;  and  that 
although  opposite  in  direction  they  are  co-ordinate  reciprocal 
factors  in  one  united  response. 

In  the  crossed  extension-reflex  this  "  reciprocal  innervation  " 
is  seen  conversely  inhibiting  the  flexors  while  causing  contrac- 
tion of  the  extensors.  This  reflex  is  well  excited  by  stimulation 
of  the  opposite  planta.  The  myograph  lever  recording  the 
contraction  of  one  of  the  hamstring  muscles,  isolated  to  sample 
the  group,  is  then  seen  to  register  a  quick  relaxation  interrupt- 


94 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Leci 


Time  in  .01' 


Electromagnet 
of  primary 
circuit 


Myograph 
Time  in  i" 


Figure  30.  —  A  and  B.  The  "  flexion-reflex  "  observed  as  reflex  contraction  (excitation)  of 
the  flexor  muscle  of  the  knee  (Fig.  30A)  and  as  reflex  relaxation  (inhibition)  of  the 
extensor  muscle  of  the  knee  (Fig.  30B).  The  afferent  nerve  stimulated  is  a  twig  of  the 
internal  saphenous  below  the  knee.  The  stimulation  is  by  a  series  of  break  induction 
currents,  the  number  and  frequency  of  which  is  shown  by  the  electromagnet  record  of 
the  breaks  and  makes  of  the  constant  current  feeding  tlie  primary  spiral  of  the  induc- 
torium  through  a  rotating  key.     The  distance  of  the  secondary  coil  from  the  primary 


Ill] 


RECIPROCAL  INNERVATION 


95 


remained  the  same  in  the  two  observations  (Figs.  30A  and  30B).  The  observation 
of  Fig.  30B  was  made  from  the  same  preparation  as  Fig.  30A  and  about  four  minutes  later, 
The  moment  of  delivery  of  the  individual  stimuli  is  marked  by  the  abscissae  on  the  myo- 
gram :  in  Fig.  30A  six  were  delivered  before  the  reflex  contraction  set  in  ;  similarly  in  Fig. 
30B,  six  were  delivered  before  the  reflex  relaxation  set  in.  The  mtensity  of  the  stimulat- 
ing shocks  was  feeble,  hence  the  relatively  long  latent  period.  Time  recorded  in  hun- 
dredth seconds  above,  in  seconds  below. 


96 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


rime  in  .01" 


Time  in 


Figure  31.  —  A  and  B.  The  "flexion-reflex"  observed  as  reflex  contraction  (excitation) 
of  the  flexor  muscle  of  the  knee  (Fig.  31A)  and  as  reflex  relaxation  (inhibition)  of  the  ex- 
tensor muscle  of  the  knee  (Fig.  31B).  Conditions  the  same  as  in  Fig.  30,  except  that 
the  secondary  coil  of  the  inductorium  is  nearer  to  primary,  and  therefore  stimulation 
more  intense.  The  latency  is  therefore  shorter  than  in  the  pair  of  observations  yielding 
Fig.  3c.     In  Fig.  31 A  the  first  three  stimuli  fall  within  the  latent  period;  in  Fig.  31B  the 


Ill] 


RECIPROCAL  INNERVATION 


97 


first  two  stimuli  only.  The  reflex  contraction  excited  is  more  vigorous  and  prolonged 
than  with  the  weaker  stimuli  of  Fig.  30.  (The  myograph  lever  at  the  top  of  its  ascent 
has  touched  the  carrier  of  the  electromagnetic  signal,  and  its  further  record  is  retarded 
until  it  begins  to  descend).  Electromagnetic  records  of  interruptions  of  constant  current 
in  primary  circuit,  and  of  time,  as  in  Figs,  30A  and  30B. 

7 


98 


THE   SIMPLE    REFLEX 


[Lect./ 


ing  the  reflex  contraction  that  until  then  had  been  in  progress j 
(Fig.  32).  The  speed  with  which  the  reflex  inhibition  occurs 
and  is  accomplished  is  much  the  same  as  that  which  reflex  con- 
traction itself  exhibits.  It  hardly  seems  slower.  But  it  is  often 
noticeable  that  the  relaxation  thus  induced  in  the  contraction 
does  not  reduce  the  contraction  to  zero  (Fig.  32).  The  relaxa- 
tion ensues  down  to  another  grade  of  contraction,  at  which 
grade  the  inhibition  often  continues  to  hold  it;  at  least,  the 
muscle  continues  to  remain  at  that  length.  In  such  cases  thcj 
contraction  is  reduced  suddenly  from  a  high  level  of  intensity' 
to  a  lower  level,  but  a  remainder  of  contraction  persists.  It! 
may  be  that  this  lower  grade  represents  another  functional  act! 
in  which  the  muscle  is  simply  adjuvant  towards  steadying  thei 
levers  for  other  muscles  which  replace  itself  in  its  previous  r61ej 


Figure  32.  —  Myograph  record  of  reflex  contraction  of  semimembranosus  induced  by  stimu  \ 
lation  (unipolar  faradization)  of  the  skin  of  the  homonymous  foot.  The  duration  of  thi  i 
stimulus  is  marked  by  the  upper  signal.  The  lower  signal  marks  the  time  of  applicatioi  4 
of  a  stimulation  (unipolar  faradization)  of  the  skin  of  the  contralateral  foot :  this  stimula  \ 
tion  caused  immediate  relaxation  of  the  contracting  hamstring  muscle,  but  the  relaxatioi  % 
did  not  proceed  beyond  a  certain  grade.     Time  is  marked  above  in  fifths  of  seconds. 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL   INHIBITION  99 

of  principal  actor.  The  condition  under  which  I  have  most 
frequently  met  it  has  been  when  the  exposed  and  freed 
tendon  of  the  semitendinosus  (dog)  has  been  attached  to  the 
myograph. 

It  is  interesting  that  when  (Fig.  33)  the  inhibiting  stimulus 
is  strong  the  relaxation  of  the  extensor  muscles  is  actually  to 
a  point  beyond  their  initial  length  obtaining  at  the  time  the 
"  crossed  extension-reflex  "  began.  The  pre-existent  "  decere- 
brate" tonus  is  inhibited  as  well  as  the  intercurrent  reflex. 
The  relaxation  is  indeed  down  to,  as  I  expressed  it  in  one  of 
my  earlier  notes,^^  the  post  mortem  length  of  the  muscle.  The 
relaxation,  if  the  crossed-reflex  stimulus  continues,  is  rapidly 
recovered  from,  and  the  interrupted  reflex  reasserts  itself 
(Figs.  33  and  34). 

Concordantly  with  these  results  examination  with  the  myo- 
graph of  the  contractions  of  the  pretibial  and  post-tibial  muscles 
of  the  frog  ^^^  during  alternate  flexor  and  extensor  strokes  of  the 
hind  limb  shows  in  many  cases,  though  not  in  all,  that  the  con- 
tractions of  the  two  antagonistic  muscles  are  not  synchronous, 
but  are  conversely  timed.  The  contraction  of  the  pretibial 
muscle  breaks  down  just  as  that  of  the  post-tibial  ensues,  and 
the  post-tibial  relaxes  just  as  the  pretibial  contracts. 

An  early  noted  and  in  various  ways  typical  example  of  this 
r61e  of  reflex  "  inhibition  "  was  that  discovered  by  E.  Hering*^ 
and  J.  Breuer**  (1868)  in  the  "  self-regulating  "  respiratory  vagus 
action.  Distension  of  the  lung  by  exciting  afferent  fibres  in  the 
pulmonary  vagus  inhibits  inspiration  and  excites  expiration.  I 
reverted  to  this  as  a  fundamental  instance  in  my  first  note  ^^  on 
the  subject.  If  we  regard  the  heart  and  ring-musculature  of  the 
arteries  as  two  antagonistic  muscles,  v.  Cyon's  *^  still  earlier  dis- 
covery (1866)  that  the  afl*erent  nerve  of  the  heart,  and  aorta 
(A.  Tschermak  and  Koster)^^^  —  from  this  point  of  view,  a 
tendon  of  the  heart  muscle  —  evokes  reflex  inhibition  of  the 
arterial  ring  musculature,  is  another  instance.  The  sugges- 
tiveness  of  these  facts  for  the  co-ordination  of  skeletal  muscles 
was  not  recognised  generally.  But  Meltzer,  the  discoverer  with 
Kronecker^^   of  the  r6le  of  inhibition   in   normal  deglutition. 


lOO  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

wrote  in  1883,^^  "Of  a  purposeful  arrangement  we  could  ex- 
pect that  a  nerve,  the  stimulation  of  which  causes  flexion, 
ought  to  contain  also  inhibitory  fibres  for  the  extensors.  Now 
such  an  arrangement  is  indeed  present  —  at  least  in  the  re- 
spiratory mechanism.  Of  the  superior  laryngeal  nerve,  of  the 
second  branch  of  the  trigeminus,  and  of  the  splanchnics,  we 
know  that  stimulation  of  their  central  end  causes  inhibition  of 
the  inspiratory  and  contraction  of  the  expiratory  muscles." 

Now  in  the  case  of  the  skeletal  muscles  of  the  mammalian 
limb,  no  efferent  nerve-fibres  appear  to  be  supplied  to  them 
which  under  stimulation  produce  inhibition  of  their  contraction. 
Such  have  been  sought  for  by  various  observers,  but  without 
success.  I  have  myself  looked  for  them  and  obtained  no  un- 
equivocal evidence  of  their  existence.  Moreover,  Verworn^^" 
has  shown  that  during  the  inhibitory  relaxation  produced  by 
the  reflex  induced  from  the  nerve  of  the  antagonistic  muscles 
the  excitability  of  the  relaxed  muscle  and  its  motor-nerve  to 
electrical  stimuli  remains  undiminished. 

Moreover,  in  the  condition  of  decerebrate  rigidity,  when  the 
elbow  is  being  kept  in  extension  by  the  heightened  tonic  action 
of  the  extensor  muscles,  their  contraction  can  be  inhibited  not 
only  by  stimulation  of  the  crossed  hind  foot,  but  by  direct 
electrical  stimulation  of  a  point  in  the  lateral  column  of  the 
transected  spinal  cord  in  the  hind  thoracic  region,  as  has  been 
shown  by  A.  Frohlich  and  myself.^^  The  inhibition  reflexly 
produced  has  therefore  its  seat  in  the  spinal  part  of  the  reflex- 
arcs.     It  is  therefore  a  central  inhibition. 

This  central  inhibition  appears  more  than  equivalent  to 
merely  arresting  the  play  of  an  excited  afferent  channel  upon 


Figure  33  (opposite).  —  Myograph  record  of  reflex  contraction  of  extensor  of  knee  interrupted 
by  areflex  inhibition  (relaxation).  The  reflex  contraction  was  induced  by  stimulation  (uni- 
polar faradization)  of  the  skin  of  the  opposite  foot:  this  stimulation  was  applied  during 
the  time  marked  by  the  lower  signal ;  its  moments  of  commencement  and  ending  are 
marked  by  abscissae  on  the  myogram.  Towards  the  height  of  the  reflex  contraction  a 
brief  stimulation  (unipolar  faradization)  was  applied  to  the  skin  of  the  foot  homonymous 
with  the  knee  extensor  yielding  the  myogram :  the  duration  of  this  inhibiting  stimulus  is 
marked  by  the  upper  signal.  The  knee  extensor  at  outset  was  in  some  tonic  contraction 
due  to  "  decerebrate  rigidity."  The  reflex  inhibition  relaxes  this  in  addition  to  inhibit- 
ing the  current  reflex  from  the  crossed  foot.     Time  is  marked  below  in  fifths  of  seconds. 


Ill] 


RECIPROCAL   INHIBITION 


lOI 


I02 


THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  34,  —  Similar  to  Fig.  ^^j  except  that  the  inhibiting  stimulus  was  weak  faradization 
applied  to  the  proximal  end  of  the  severed  "  hamstring  nerve." 


the  motor  centre.  Were  that  all,  the  phenomenon  should  re- 
semble the  effect  of  suddenly  stopping  the  stimulation  of  the 
afferent  nerve  causing  the  reflex.  What  happens  is  often  not 
like  that;  the  arrest  is  more  rapid.  The  "after-discharge," 
whatever  its  seat,  can  be  at  once  arrested  by  the  inhibition 
(Fig.  35).  The  "after-discharge"  of  a  centre,  with  its  con- 
comitant persistence  of  contraction  of  muscles,  might  well  be 
disadvantageous  to  the  organism.      That  it  is  rapidly  arrested 


Ill] 


RECIPROCAL  INHIBITION 


103 


by  the  inhibitory  side  of  a  succeeding  reflex,  is  an  adaptation 
which  facilitates  the  successive  interchange  of  reflexes.  The 
inhibition  can  arrest  some  forms  of  clonic  spasm  arising  during 
experimentation  (Fig.  36). 

The  motor  neurones  of  the  flexor  muscles  of  the  hind  limb 
can  be  excited  to  the  clonic  discharge  characteristic  of  the 
scratch-reflex  at  a  time  when  the  flexion-reflex  is  inhibited 
from  employing  them.     When  the  scratch-reflex  is  in  progress 


Figure  35.—  Myrograph  records  of  reflex  contractions  of  the  extensor  of  the  knee  in  'decere- 
brate "  cat.  The  exciting  stimulus  was,  in  the  observation  reproduced  on  the  left  of  the 
figure,  a  brief  compression  —  lasting  less  than  a  second  —  of  a  digit  of  the  contralateral 
foot.  After  this  stimulus  had  been  given  and  discontinued,  and  while  the  after-discharge 
of  the  reflex  was  still  in  progress,  the  proximal  end  of  a  branch  of  the  severed  hamstring 
nerve  was  stimulated  by  faradization  for  about  a  quarter  of  a  second.  The  time  of  this 
inhibiting  stimulus  is  marked  by  the  sigjnal.  The  reflex  after-discharge  is  seen  to  have 
been  at  once  inhibited  and  in  this  case  not  to  have  returned. 

The  observation  reproduced  on  the  right  was  from  the  same  experiment,  but  later  ;  in  it 
the  stimulation  exciting  the  reflex  contraction  was  faradization  of  the  proximal  end  of  a 
twig  of  the  internal  saphenous  of  the  contralateral  leg.  This  stimulation  lasted  about 
two  fifths  of  a  second  or  less.  Its  cessation  was  quickly  succeeded  by  faradization  of  the 
proximal  end  of  a  branch  of  the  severed  hamstring  nerve  as  in  the  previous  observation. 
The  signal  marks  the  time  of  this  inhibiting  stimulation.  The  after-discharge  of  the  con- 
traction reflex  is  cut  short  as  before.     Time  is  marked  below  in  fifths  of  seconds. 


104 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


Figure  36.  —  Myogram  of  convulsive  twitching  of  semitendinosus  in  a  "  spinal"  dog.  The 
spasms  are  reduced  and  temporarily  suspended  by  stimulation  (faradization)  of  the  proxi- 
mal end  of  a  branch  of  the  internal  saphenous  nerve  of  the  contralateral  leg.  The  time 
of  application  of  the  inhibiting  stimulus  is  shown  on  the  signal  line  below.  Time  is 
marked  above  in  seconds. 


it  is  more  difficult  to  excite  a  "  flexion-reflex,"  and  vice  versa. 
One  reflex  seems  to  be  precluded  from  acting  on  a  motor 
neurone  at  a  time  when  another  and  difi"erent  reflex  is  employ- 
ing it.^^  The  preclusion  of  the  motor  neurone  from  one  reflex 
while  it  is  still  left  open  to  it  to  respond  to  other  reflexes  ap- 
pears to  be  one  of  the  services  of  inhibition  to  the  organism. 
The  motor  neurone  itself  seems  not  the  actual  seat  of  the  in- 


Ill]  RECIPROCAL  INHIBITION  105 

hibition,  for  if  so,  it  would  be  inhibited  for  all  reflexes;  un- 
less the  motor  neurone  is  functionally  divisible,  and  one  part 
of  it,  e.  g.,  one  set  of  dendrites,  can  be  inhibited  at  a  time  when 
another  is  not.  The  seat  of  the  inhibition  appears,  therefore, 
with  some  likelihood,  to  lie  neither  in  the  afferent  neurone 
proper  nor  in  the  efferent  neurone  proper,  but  in  an  internun- 
cial  mechanism  —  synapse  or  neurone  —  between  them.  I  say 
"  neurone  proper,"  meaning  to  exclude  from  that  term  the 
synapse,  although  in  a  synapse  the  neurone  terminals  are 
included. 

The  striking  correspondence  observed  {y,  j.)  between  the 
reflex  inhibition  and  the  reflex  contraction,  when  examined  in 
one  and  the  same  type-reflex,  allows  the  inference  that  the 
nerve-fibres  from  the  receptive  field  of  the  reflex  each  divide 
in  the  spinal  cord  into  end-branches  {e.g.^  collaterals),  one  set 
of  which,  when  the  nerve-fibre  is  active,  produces  excitation, 
while  another  set,  when  the  nerve-fibre  is  active,  produces  inhi- 
bition.2^>  ^^  The  single  afferent  nerve-fibre  would  therefore  in 
regard  to  one  set  of  its  terminal  branches  be  specifically  excitor^ 
and  in  regard  to  another  set  of  its  central  endings  be  specifically 
inhibitory.  It  would,  in  this  respect,  be  duplex  centrally  (Fig.  37). 
There  is  analogy  between  the  structural  arrangement  for  reflex 
reciprocal  innervation  and  that  of  Astacus  claw,  if  it  be  sup- 
posed that  the  individual  nerve-fibres  of  the  crayfish-claw  prepa- 
ration dichotomise,  one  division  of  the  nerve-fibre  passing  to 
the  closing  muscle,  the  other  to  the  opening  muscle;  so  that 
one  division  of  the  fibre  exerts  the  excitor  action,  the  other 
the  well-known  inhibitory,  studied  by  Richet,^  Biedermann,^^ 
Piotrowski,^*^  and  others. 

In  denoting  one  set  of  central  terminations  of  an  afferent  arc 
"  specifically  inhibitory^'  it  is  here  meant  that  by  no  mere  change 
in  intensity  or  mode  of  stimulation  can  they  be  brought  to  yield 
any  other  effect  than  inhibition.  But  the  fact  that  stimulation 
of  a  single  set  of  afferent  arcs,  namely  a  single  small  afferent 
nerve,  excites  frequently  a  reflex  movement  of  alternating  direc- 
tion in  which,  for  instance  at  the  knee,  extension  succeeds  pri- 
mary flexion,  shows  that  a  change  of  internal  conditions  may 


io6  THE  SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

presumably  convert  an  intraspinal  connection  that  under  the 
primary  conditions  is  inhibitory  into  one  that  under  later  super- 
vening conditions  becomes  excitatory.  The  fact  that  under 
certain  forms  of  cerebral  action  true  antagonistic  muscles  can 
be  thrown  synchronously  into  contraction,  points  to  the  same 
limitation  of  the  term  "  specific  "  in  this  connection.  Further, 
there  is  the  intraspinal  action  of  strychnine. 

There  is  the  long  recognized  fact  that  under  strychnine 
practically  all  the  skeletal  muscles  of  the  body  may  be  reflexly 
thrown  into  contraction  simultaneously,  and  this  is  obviously 
inclusive  of,  and  was  proved  for,  antagonistic  muscles.^^  Evi- 
dently strychnine  in  some  way  must  alter  or  obscure  reciprocal 
innervation.  I  have  furnished  (1892)  tracings  showing  that  the 
pretibial  and  post-tibial  muscles  of  the  frog,  although  in  normal 
reflex  movements  so  frequently  exhibiting  concurrent  contrac- 
tion and  relaxation  in  the  two  groups  reciprocally,  under 
strychnine  reveal  in  the  double  myogram  perfectly  synchronous 
contraction  in  both  groups.^^ 

Such  a  result  may  be  explicable  in  several  ways.  In  order 
to  discover  what  the  nature  of  the  change  wrought  by  strychnine 
really  is,  there  have  to  be  fulfilled  in  the  test  experiments  cer- 
tain conditions  which  not  every  preparation  of  antagonistic 
muscles  can  supply.  Muscles  acting  over  two  joints  are  to  be 
avoided  in  such  a  test.  Thus  the  gastrocnemius  of  the  frog  ex- 
tends the  ankle  but  flexes  the  knee ;  it  antagonizes  the  action 
of  the  pretibial  muscles  which  flex  the  ankle,  but  since  flexion 
of  the  knee  so  commonly  accompanies  flexion  of  the  ankle,  it 
is  synergic  with  the  pretibial  muscles  in  the  great  flexion-reflex 
that  draws  up  the  limb.  If  it  acts  synchronously  with  pretibial 
muscles  under  strychnine,  we  are  still  left  in  a  dilemma  as  to 
whether  the  co-ordinate  reciprocal  action  at  the  ankle  is  essen- 
tially destroyed,  or  whether  a  reflex  attempt  to  flex  the  knee 
has  not  been  simply  added  to  it  under  a  lowering  of  the  intra- 
spinal resistances.  And  this  dilemma  is  the  greater  in  that  the 
aflerent  nerves  and  surfaces  used  for  exciting  reflexes  contain 
admixed  afferent  channels,  some  exciting  contraction  in  one 
group  of  the  opposed  muscles,  and  some  exciting  contraction  in 


™in 


II]  ACTION   OF   STRYCHNINE  107 

the  other.  Thus  in  the  afferent  nerves  from  the  foot,  both  in  the 
dog^  and  the  frog,^^  there  are  commingled  with  fibres  which 
excite  the  flexor  muscles  those  which  excite  the  extensor  muscles 
—  witness  the  extensor-thrust  and  flexion-reflex,  both  elicitable 
from  the  dog* s  foot,  and  Baglioni's  extension-reflex  of  the  leg 
and  the  flexion-reflex,  both  elicitable  from  the  frog's  leg.  That 
the  extensor  muscles  of  the  Hmb  should  under  strychnine  be 
thrown  into  contraction  synchronously  with  the  flexors  in  these 
cases  might  be  due  either  to  the  two  reflexes  being  elicited 
together  when  spinal  resistance  has  been  lowered,  or  to  a  con- 
version of  the  inhibition  part  of  one  reflex  into  an  excitation. 
And  in  this  latter  case  it  is  still  left  undecided  whether  the 
extension  under  strychnine  is  due  to  prepotent  extensor-reflex 
with  its  accompanying  flexor  inhibition  changed  into  excitation, 
or  whether  it  is  the  flexion-reflex  which  is  changed  conversely. 

On  similar  grounds  the  "  spontaneous  "  convulsions  due  to 
strychnine  afford  no  deeper  insight  into  the  problem.  These 
"spontaneous"  convulsions  are  really  reflex  (Stannius,  CI. 
Bernard,  H.  E.  Hering,  and  others)  in  the  sense  that  they 
originate  in  the  afferent  arcs ;  and  in  the  convulsive  movements 
antagonistic  muscles  contract  simultaneously.  But  the  difficulty 
here  again  is,  that  the  reflex  source  may,  and  probably  does, 
operate  in  many  afferent  arcs  concurrently.  Some  of  these  arcs 
excite  extensor  muscles  normally,  while  others  excite  flexors. 
The  simultaneous  contraction  of  both  flexors  and  extensors 
might  thus  be  naturally  explicable  by  lowered  spinal  resistance, 
both  sets  of  reflexes  being  equally  induced  together,  or  the 
explanation  might  be  of  an  alternative  kind,  such  as  suggested 
above.  On  the  former  view  the  reciprocal  innervation  of  antag- 
onistic muscles  would  merely  be  obscured  by  a  simultaneous 
double  reflex;  on  the  latter  a  more  profound  alteration  would 
have  taken  place.  The  occurrence  and  form  of  the  convulsion 
fail  to  decide  among  these  possibilities. 

Conditions  for  determining  the  nature  of  the  action  that  really 
occurs  seem  offered,  however,  in  certain  instances.  Thus,  in  the 
hind  limb  of  the  cat  we  have  two  afferent  nerves  which  never, 
under  any  normal  conditions  ^^  in  my  experience,  yield  as  their 


io8 


THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX 


[Lect. 


primary  reflex  in  the  vasti-crureus  muscle  any  action  but  relaxa- 
tion;  in  other  words,  they,  without  exception,  produce  reflex 
inhibition  of  that  muscle.  To  suppose  that  these  nerves  contain 
afferent  fibres  which  evoke  reflexly  at  the  knee  any  action  other 


Figure  37.  —  Diagram  indicating  connections  and  actions  of  two  afferent  spinal  root-cells,  9. 
and  a',  in  regard  to  their  reflex  influence  on  the  extensor  and  flexor  muscles  of  the  two 
knees,  a,  root-cell  afferent  from  skin  below  knee ;  a',  root-cell  afferent  from  flexor 
muscle  of  knee,  /.  *.,  in  hamstring  nerve  ;  «  and  «',  efferent  neurones  to  the  extensor 
muscles  of  the  knee,  left  and  right ;  5  and  6',  efferent  neurones  to  the  flexor  muscles ;  E 
and  E', extensor  muscles;  F  and  F',  flexor  muscles.  The  "schalt-zellen"  (v.  Monakow) 
probable  between  the  afferent  and  efferent  root-cells  are  for  simplicity  omitted.  The  sign 
+  indicates  that  at  the  synapse  which  it  marks  the  afferent  fibre  a  (and  a')  excites  the 
motor  neurone  to  discharging  activity,  whereas  the  sign  —  indicates  that  at  the  synapse 
which  it  marks  the  afferent  fibre  a  (and  a')  inhibits  the  discharging  activity  of  the  motor 
neurones.  The  effect  of  strychnine  and  of  tetanus  toxin  is  to  convert  the  minus  sign  into 
plus  sign. 


Ill]  ACTION   OF   STRYCHNINE  109 

than  flexion  would  be  mere  hypothesis.  These  two  nerves  are 
the  internal  saphenous  in  its  course  below  the  knee,  and  the 
hamstring  nerve,  coming  from  the  flexor  muscles  of  the  knee. 
Further,  the  vasti-criireus  is  a  single-joint  muscle,  and  unlike 
the  rest  of  the  quadriceps  extensor  of  the  thigh  is  not  a  flexor 
of  the  hip ;  therefore  contraction  in  it  cannot  mean  merely  its 
participation  in  the  synergy  of  the  flexion-reflex  itself,  which 
includes  flexion  at  the  hip.  A  reflex  preparation  suited  for  ex- 
amining the  action  of  strychnine  on  reciprocal  innervation  can, 
therefore,  be  obtained  in  the  hind  limb  by  severing,  in  the  de- 
cerebrate animal  for  instance,  the  following  nerves :  the  external 
popliteal,  the  internal  popliteal,  the  obturator  and  pudic  in  the 
pelvis,  the  superior  gluteal,  the  external  and  cutaneous  divisions 
of  the  anterior  crural  and  the  hamstring  nerve.  The  last  named 
is  ligated  and  cut  close  to  its  entrance  in  the  muscles,  so  that 
its  central  end  can  be  stimulated.  A  branch  of  the  internal 
saphenous  nerve  below  the  knee  is  also  prepared  for  stimulation 
of  its  central  end.  When  this  is  done  it  is  found  that  no  change^ 
in  intensity  or  other  conditions  of  excitation  of  the  afferent  s 
nerve  ever  provokes  anything  but  inhibition  of  the  extensor 
of  the  knee,  but  a  small  dose  of  strychnine  at  once  transmutes 
the  inhibitory  eff*ect  into  an  excitation  eff*ect^^  Reflex  con-  " 
traction  is  obtained  in  place  of  reflex  relaxation.  If  small  doses^ 
are  carefully  graded,  it  is  possible  to  see  a  state  in  which  the  ' 
reflex  relaxation  is  diminished  but  is  not  replaced  by  excita- 
tion. This  phenomenon  shows  well  how  little  competent  is  the 
view  of  lowered  spinal  resistance  to  really  explain  the  action  of 
strychnine ;  for  at  this  stage  the  stimulated  arc  that  normally 
acts  on  the  extensor  muscle  by  inhibition  is  less  able  to  aff"ect  it 
than  before,  so  that  on  the  spinal  resistance  view  the  resistance 
at  this  stage  is  actually  heightened. 

A  similar  conversion  of  inhibitory  effect  into  excitatory 
is  produced  more  gradually  but  not  less  potently  by  tetanus 
toxin.304 

This  conversion  sets  in  before  and  under  smaller  doses  of 
strychnine  or  toxin  than  are  required  to  produce  the  convulsive 
seizures  characteristic  of  strychnine  poisoning,  or  general  tetanus. 


no  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

The  transformation  of  effect  by  strychnine  holds  good  not 
only  for  the  nerves  above  mentioned  but  for  skin-stimuli,  and  also 
for  those  skin  points  remote  from  the  hind  limb  itself,  which  pro- 
voke reflex  inhibition  of  the  test  muscle.  For  instance,  in  the 
case  of  the  knee-extensor  as  test  muscle,  the  skin  of  the  fore  paws. 

The  conversion  of  inhibitory  effect  into  excitation  effect  by 
strychnine  is  more  easily  obtained  in  the  case  of  some  nerves 
than  of  others.  In  the  instances  of  the  nerves  above  mentioned 
the  conversion  is  least  facile,  i.  e.y  requires  larger  doses  or  longer 
time  for  development,  in  the  case  of  the  hamstring  nerve,  than  in 
the  others.  The  inhibitory  effect  belonging  to  that  nerve  is 
readily  lessened  by  the  strychnine,  but  its  actual  replacement 
by  excitation  effect,  e.g.^  contraction  of  knee-extensor,  not  only 
requires  larger  doses  of  strychnine,  but  is  even  then  phasic  rather 
than  continuous.  When  this  nerve  is  tested  by  stimulation  at 
regular  short  intervals  during  one  of  these  phasic  periods,  it  can 
be  seen  that,  starting  from  the  phase  in  which  it  still  evokes  in- 
hibition little,  or  perhaps  not  at  all,  less  obviously  than  in  the 
normal  state,  its  inhibitory  effect  then  becomes  progressively  less, 
until  it  is  replaced  by  excitation  effect  (contraction),  at  first  mild, 
later  violent.     This  periodic  phase  will  repeat  itself  many  times. 

The  conversion  of  inhibition  effect  as  thus  tested  on  the 
knee-extensor  might  be  attributable  to  the  afferent  nerves  stimu- 
lated containing  two  kinds  of  afferent  fibres  admixed,  one  kind 
causing  reflex  contraction  of  the  muscle,  the  other  kind  reflex 
inhibition.  Strychnine  might,  by  augmenting  the  action  of  the 
former  or  by  depressing  the  action  of  the  latter,  change  the 
effect  of  stimulation  of  the  mixed  nerve.  But  the  latter  fibres 
would  be  expected  to  be  associated  in  their  action  with  —  or,  as 
urged  above,  to  be  even  the  self-same  fibers  which  evoke  —  con- 
traction of  the  flexor  muscles.  Now  there  is  at  the  stage  of 
strychnization,  at  which  the  change  of  inhibitory  into  excitatory 
effect  occurs,  no  trace  of  any  paralysis  or  even  depression  of 
the  flexor  contractions.  The  protagonist  and  the  antagonist 
muscles  are  thrown  together  into  synchronous  contraction  as 
an  effect  of  strychnine.  This  and  other  considerations  ap- 
pear  to    me   to   weigh   against   explaining   the   conversion   of 


Ill]  ACTION   OF   STRYCHNINE  in 

inhibition  effect  into  excitation  effect  by  the  hypothesis  of  reflex 
antagonistic  sets  of  fibres,  oppositely  poisoned  centrally,  com- 
mingled in  these  afferent  nerves.  Moreover,  when  a  hamstring 
muscle  is  taken  as  the  test  muscle,  a  similar  conversion  of 
inhibition  into  excitation  (contraction)  by  strychnine  is  seen 
under  the  crossed  extension-reflex. .  This  reflex,  elicitable 
through  the  skin  or  various  afferent  nerves  of  the  contralateral 
hind  limb,  normally  excites  the  knee-extensor  to  contraction  and 
inhibits  the  hamstrings,  the  knee-flexors.  Under  strychnine  its 
reflex  inhibition  of  the  hamstring  muscle  is  converted  into  reflex 
excitation  (contraction)  of  that  muscle.  The  observations,  as 
they  stand  at  present,  incline  me  to  the  inference  that  the  action 
of  the  alkaloid  is  to  convert  in  the  spinal  cord  the  process  of 
inhibition  —  whatever  that  may  essentially  be  —  into  the  process 
of  excitation  —  whatever  that  may  essentially  be.^  The  reflex 
nexus  was  pre-existent,  but  the  effect  across  it  was  signalized  by 
a  different  sign,  namely  minus  prior  to  the  strychnine  or  tetanus 
toxin,  instead  oipliis,  as  afterward  (See  Fig.  37). 

The  action  of  the  toxin  in  respect  to  inhibition  resembles 
that  of  strychnine  closely  in  several  ways.  Thus,  in  the  stages 
of  the  disease  in  which  the  tetanus  is  still  **  local "  and  manifested 
in  one  limb,  namely  that  (^e.  g.,  the  hind  limb)  which  received 
the  toxin  injection,  the  toxin  early  converts  into  excitation  the 
reflex  inhibition  of  the  extensor  muscles,  normally  obtainable 
from  the  internal  saphenous  nerve,  but  that  obtainable  from  the 
peroneal  and  popliteal  nerves,  and  from  the  hamstring  nerve, 
remains  unreversed,  though  the  strength  of  the  inhibitory  effect 
of  these  nerves  may  be  very  distinctly  less  than  normal.  Later, 
as  the  condition  progresses,  the  inhibitory  effect  normally  be- 

1  From  the  predominance  of  extension  as  a  reflex  in  the  hind  limb  of  the  *  spinal  * 
frog  under  strychnine,  flexion  predominating  normally,  Cushny  has  recently  (3d 
Edition  of  Textbook  of  Pharmacology  and  Therapeutics,  Philadelphia,  1903) 
argued  much  as  I  did  (Joum.  of  Physiology,  vol.  xiii,  1892),  also  from  experiments 
on  the  frog,  that  strychnine  acts  as  a  destroyer  of  reciprocal  innervation.  The 
hind  limb  of  the  frog,  owing  to  the  number  of  double-joint  muscles,  is,  as  said  in  the 
text,  not  a  preparation  wherewith  it  seems  possible  to  definitely  test  this  argument 
or  inference.  But  the  evidence  obtained  from  more  suitable  preparations  fully 
bears  out,  as  the  text  shows,  the  earlier  inferences  drawn  by  Cushny  and  myself 
regarding  the  frog.     [October,  1905.     C.  S.  S.] 


112  THE   SIMPLE   REFLEX  [Lect. 

longing  to  the  peroneal  and  popliteal  nerves  becomes  actually 
reversed  into  excitation.  Finally,  even  that  of  the  hamstring 
nerve  itself  is  reversed.  This  is  the  same  sequence  of  effect 
pursued  by  progressive  increase  of  dosage  of  strychnine. 

One  difference  that  seems  apparent  between  the  action  of 
the  tetanus  toxin  and  the  strychnine  in  these  observations  is 
that  in  the  relatively  slow  progress  of  the  tetanus  it  is  easy  to 
note  the  stage  in  which  the  conversion  of  the  inhibition  effect 
into  excitation  effect  has  occurred,  while  there  is  yet  none  of 
that  obvious  lowering  of  the  threshold  of  reflex  reaction  which 
early  marks  the  course  of  strychnine  poisoning,  and  has  been 
drawn  attention  to  by  many  observers. 

In  experiments  on  the  hind  limb,  I  have  usually  introduced 
the  toxin  into  the  sciatic  trunk  well  below  the  hamstring  branch, 
more  rarely  into  the  hamstring  nerve  as  well,  or  alone.  I  have 
found  the  inhibitory  effect  of  the  internal  saphenous  nerve 
(stimulated  in  its  course  below  the  knee)  converted  to  excitation 
in  forty-eight  hours  from  the  time  of  inoculation.  In  the  gradual 
progress  of  the  condition,  I  have  several  times  found  the  ham- 
string nerve  produce  slight  inhibition  of  the  extensor  if  the  initial 
posture  taken  at  the  knee  be  extension,  and  yet  produce  distinct 
excitation  of  the  extensor  if  the  initial  posture  taken  at  the  knee 
be  flexion.  This  recalls  the  results  of  v.  Uexkiill  in  Ophioglypha 
and  Echinus.*-®^* 

The  conversion  of  inhibition  into  excitation  by  tetanus  toxin 
is  demonstrable,  as  is  that  by  strychnine,  with  the  reflexes  of  the 
fore  limb  as  well  as  with  those  of  the  hind  limb,  and  in  the 
**  decerebrate  "  animal  as  well  as  in  the  merely  **  spinal.  " 

We  can  understand  what  havoc  such  a  change  must  work  in  the 
co-ordinative  mechanisms.^^  The  observed  difference  between 
the  facility  with  which  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  convert  the  in- 
hibition by  the  hamstring  nerve  into  excitation,  and  that  with  which 
they  convert  the  inhibition  of  the  other  limb-nerves  mentioned, 
does  not  seem  referable  to  a  different  action  on  muscular  affer- 
ents  and  cutaneous  afferents  respectively.  Stimulation  of  the 
central  end  of  the  vasto-cnireus  nerve  evokes  normally  inhibition 
of  the  hamstrings  of  the  opposite  limb,  but  under  strychnine  it 


ix 


ACTION   OF  STRYCHNINE  113 

evokes  their  contraction.  In  that  case,  therefore,  the  strychnine 
converts  with  facility  the  inhibition  by  a  muscular  afferent  into 
excitation,  just  as  with  the  skin  nerves  mentioned. 

That  strychnine  and  tetanus  can  convert  a  central  inhibition 
into  an  excitation,  and  that  the  various  normal  reflex  spinal  in- 
hibitions show  differences  one  from  another  in  the  ease  with 
which  they  undergo  conversion  into  excitation  makes  the  syn- 
chronous excitation  of  antagonistic  muscles  in  certain  willed 
actions  less  difficult  to  understand.  Vasodepressor  reflexes 
under  chloral  (v.  Cyon),  chloroform  (Bayliss),^*^  etc.,  change 
into  vasoconstrictor  under  curare,  morphia,  etc.  But  the  re- 
versal does  not  appear  to  occur  with  equal  facility  in  all 
afferent  nerves  alike.  It  is  stated  to  be  impossible  to  obtain 
any  vascular  reflex,  but  a  depressant  one  from  the  "  depressor  " 
nerve.  This  nerve,  arising  in  the  heart  (v.  Cyon)*^  and  aorta 
(Koster  and  A.  Tschermak),^^^  may  in  a  sense  be  considered 
the  afferent  nerve  of  the  muscle  antagonistic  to  the  ring  muscu- 
lature of  the  arteries,  namely,  the  muscle  whose  tonus  it  reflexly 
depresses.  It  is  in  that  way  comparable  with  the  afferent 
nerve  of  the  hamstring  muscles  in  relation  to  the  extensors  of 
the  knee.  The  depressor  action  of  the  hamstring  nerve  on 
the  knee-extensor  seems,  as  just  said,  in  my  experience,  particu- 
larly resistant  to  conversion  from  inhibition  into  excitation  by 
strychnine. 

In  examples  of  reciprocal  innervation  drawn  from  lowlier 
organisms  and  visceral  organs  we  find  the  inhibition  a  periph- 
eral phenomenon,  that  is,  with  its  seat  outside  the  central  nervous 
system.  On  the  other  hand,  in  examples  drawn  from  higher 
organisms  and  skeletal  movements,  the  inhibition  is  a  central 
phenomenon,  e.g.^  intraspinal.  The  same  considerations  which 
traced  the  line  of  adaptation  in  placing  the  seat  of  the  refrac- 
tory phase  of  spinal  reflexes  intraspinally  between  the  ending 
of  receptive  neurone  and  the  commencement  of  motor  neurone, 
apply  here  to  central  inhibition.  The  significance  of  the  cen- 
tralization of  these  processes  of  refractory  phase  and  reciprocal 
inhibition  seems  the  same  as  we  may  infer  for  the  centrality  of 
the  central  nervous  system  itself  {vide  infi-a^  Lecture  IX). 

S 


114  INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 


LECTURE    IV 

INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES 

\ 
Argument:  The  "simple  reflex"  a  convenient  but  artificial  abstraction. 
Compounding  of  reflexes.  The  principle  of  the  common  path.  Rela- 
tive aperiodicity  of  the  final  common  path.  Afferent  arcs  which  use 
the  same  final  common  path  to  different  effect  have  successive  but 
not  simultaneous  use  of  it.  "  Allied  "  reflexes.  Allied  reflexes  act 
harmoniously,  are  capable  of  simultaneous  combination,  and  in  many 
cases  reinforce  one  another's  action  on  the  final  common  path. 
"Antagonistic"  reflexes.  Alliance  or  coalition  occurs  between  (i) 
individual  reflexes  belonging  to  the  same  "  type-reflex,"  (2)  certain 
reflexes  originated  by  receptors  of  different  species  but  situate  in  the 
same  region  of  Surface,  (3)  certain  reflexes  belonging  to  propriocep- 
tive organs  secondarily  excited  by  reflexes  initiated  at  the  body- 
surface  (the  three  fields  of  reception,  extero-ceptive,  intero-ceptive, 
and  proprio-ceptive),  (4)  certain  reflexes  initiated  from  widely  separate 
but  functionally  interconnected  body-regions.  Alliance  between  re- 
flexes exemplified  in  inhibitory  actions  as  well  as  in  excitatory.  An- 
tagonisitc  reflexes  interfere,  one  reflex  deferring,  interrupting,  or  cutting 
short  another,  or  precluding  the  latter  altogether  from  taking  effect 
on  the  final  common  path.  Intraspinal  seat  of  the  interference. 
Compound  reflexes  may  interfere  in  part.  The  place  ( ?  synapse) 
where  convergent  afferent  paths  impinge  on  a  common  path  consti- 
tutes a  mechanism  of  co-ordination.  The  convergence  of  afferent 
paths  to  form  common  paths  occurs  with  great  frequency  in  the 
central  nervous  system.  A  question  whether  any  reflexes  are  in  the 
intact  organism  wholly  neutral  one  to  another. 

%' 

We  have  hitherto  dealt  with  reflex  reactions  under  the  guise 
of  a  convenient  but  artificial  abstraction,  —  the  simple  reflex. 
That  is  to  say,  we  have  fixed  our  attention  on  the  reaction  of  a 
reflex-arc  as  if  it  were  that  of  an  isolable  and  isolated  mechanism, 
for  whose  function  the  presence  of  other  parts  of  the  nervous 
system  and  of  other  arcs  might  be  negligible  and  wholly  indif- 
ferent. This  is  improbable.  The  nervous  system  functions  as 
a  whole.  Physiological  and  histological  analysis  finds  it  con- 
nected   throughout   its   whole   extent.      Donaldson   opens   his 


w 


PRINCIPLE  OF  THE   COMMON   PATH  115 


description  of  it  with  the  remark:  "A  group  of  nerve-cells  dis- 
connected from  the  other  nerve-tissues  of  the  body,  as  muscles 
and  glands  are  disconnected  from  each  other,  would  be  without 
physiological  significance.**  A  reflex  reaction,  even  in  a  "  spinal 
animal "  where  the  soHdarity  of  the  nervous  system  has  been  so 
trenchantly  mutilated,  is  always  in  fact  a  reaction  conditioned 
not  by  one  reflex-arc  but  by  many.  A  reflex  detached  from 
the  general  nervous  condition  is  hardly  realizable. 

The  compounding  together  of  reflexes  is  therefore  a  main 
probleni  in  nervous  co-ordination.  For  this  problem  it  is  im- 
portant to  recognize  a  feature  in  the  architecture  of  the  gray- 
centred  (synaptic)  nervous  system  which  may  be  termed  "  t/te 
principle  of  the  common  path^^^  If  we  regard  the  nervous 
system  of  any  higher  organism  from  the  broad  point  of  view  a 
salient  feature  in  its  scheme  of  construction  is  the  following. 

At  the  commencement  of  every  reflex-arc  is  a  receptive 
neurone  extending  from  the  receptive  surface  to  the  central 
nervous  organ.  This  neurone  forms  the  sole  avenue  which 
impulses  generated  at  its  receptive  point  can  use  whithersoever 
be  their  destination.  This  neurone  is  therefore  a  path  exclu- 
sive to  the  impulses  generated  at  its  own  receptive  point,  and 
other  receptive  points  than  its  own  cannot  employ  it.  A  single 
receptive  point  may  play  reflexly  upon  quite  a  number  of  differ- 
ent efl*ector  organs.  It  may  be  connected  through  its  reflex 
path  with  many  muscles  and  glands  in  many  different  regions. 
Yet  all  its  reflex-arcs  spring  from  the  one  single  shank  or 
stem,  i.  e.^  from  the  one  afferent  neurone  which  conducts  from 
the  receptive  point  at  the  periphery  into  the  central  nervous 
organ. 

But  at  the  termination  of  every  reflex-arc  we  find  a  final 
neurone,  the  ultimate  conductive  link  to  an  effector  organ, 
(muscle  or  gland).  This  last  link  in  the  chain,  e.  g,  the  motor 
neurone,  differs  obviously  in  one  important  respect  from  the 
first  link  of  the  chain.  It  does  not  subserve  exclusively  impulses 
generated  at  one  single  receptive  source,  but  receives  impulses 
from  many  receptive  sources  situate  in  many  and  various  regions 
of  the  body.     It  is  the  sole  path  which  all  impulses,  no  matter 


ii6  INTERACTION  BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

whence  they  come,  must  travel  if  they  are  to  act  on  the  muscle- 
fibres  to  which  it  leads. 

Therefore,  while  the  receptive  neurone  forms  a  private  path 
exclusively  serving  impulses  of  one  source  only,  the  final  or 
efferent  neurone  is,  so  to  say,  a  public  path,  common  to  impulses 
arising  at  any  of  many  sources  of  reception.  A  receptive  field, 
e,  g.y  an  area  of  skin,  is  analyzable  into  receptive  points.  One 
and  the  same  effector  organ  stands  in  reflex  connection  not 
only  with  many  individual  receptive  points  but  even  with  many 
various  receptive  fields.  Reflexes  generated  in  manifold  sense- 
organs  can  pour  their  influence  into  one  and  the  same  muscle. 
Thus  a  limb-muscle  is  the  terminus  ad  quem  of  many  reflex- 
arcs  arising  in  many  various  parts  of  the  body.  Its  motor- 
nerve  is  a  path  common  to  all  the  reflex-arcs  which  reach  that 
muscle  {cf.  infra^  Fig.  44,  p.  148). 

Reflex-arcs  show,  therefore,  the  general  features  that  the 
initial  neurone  of  each  is  a  private  path  exclusively  belonging  to 
a  single  receptive  point  (or  small  group  of  points) ;  and  that  finally 
the  arcs  embouch  into  a  path  leading  to  an  effector  organ ;  and 
that  their  final  path  is  common  to  all  receptive  points  whereso- 
ever they  may  lie  in  the  body,  so  long  as  they  have  connection 
with  the  effector  organ  in  question.  Before  finally  converging 
upon  the  motor  neurone  the  arcs  converge  to  some  degree. 
Their  private  paths  embouch  upon  internuncial  paths  common 
in  various  degree  to  groups  of  private  paths.  The  terminal  path 
may,  to  distinguish  it  from  internuncial  common  paths,  be  called 
the  final  common  path.  The  motor  nerve  to  a  muscle  is  a 
collection  of  final  common  paths. 

Certain  consequences  result  from  this  arrangement.  One  of 
these  seems  the  preclusion  of  essential  qualitative  difference 
between  nerve-impulses  arising  in  different  afferent  nerves.  If 
two  conductors  have  a  tract  in  common,  there  can  hardly  be 
essential  qualitative  difference  between  their  modes  of  conduc- 
tion ;  and  the  final  common  paths  must  be  capable  of  respond- 
ing with  different  rhythms  which  different  conductors  impress 
upon  it.  It  must  be  to  a  certain  degree  aperiodic.  If  its  dis- 
charge be  a  rhythmic  process,  as  from  many  considerations  it 


IV]  PRINCIPLE   OF  THE   COMMON   PATH  117 

appears  to  be,  the  frequency  of  its  own  rhythm  must  be  capable 
of  being  at  least  as  high  as  that  of  the  highest  frequency  of  any 
of  the  afferent  arcs  that  play  upon  it ;  and  it  must  be  able  also 
to  reproduce  the  characters  of  the  slowest.^ 

A  second  consequence  is  that  each  receptor  being  dependent 
for  final  communication  with  its  effector  organ  upon  a  path  not 
exclusively  its  own  but  common  to  it  with  certain  other  recep- 
tors, such  nexus  necessitates  successive  and  not  simultaneous 
use  of  the  common  path  by  various  receptors  using  it  to  different 
or  opposed  effect.  When  two  receptors  are  stimulated  simultane- 
ously, each  of  the  receptors  tending  to  evoke  reflex  action  that 
for  its  end-effect  employs  the  same  final  common  path  but  em- 
ploys it  in  a  different  way  from  the  other,  one  reflex  appears 
without  the  other.  The  result  is  this  reflex  or  that  reflex,  but 
not  the  two  together. ^^^  Excitation  of  the  central  end  of  the 
afferent  root  of  the  eighth  or  seventh  cervical  nerve  of  the 
monkey  evokes  reflexly  in  the  same  individual  animal  sometimes 
flexion  at  elbow,  sometimes  extension.  If  the  excitation  be 
preceded  by  excitation  of  the  first  thoracic  root  the  result  is 
usually  extension :  if  preceded  by  excitation  of  the  sixth  cer- 
vical root  it  is  usually  flexion.  Yet  though  the  same  root  may 
thus  be  made  to  evoke  reflex  contraction  of  the  flexors  or  of  the 
extensors,  it  does  not,  in  my  experience,  evoke  contraction  in 
both  flexors  and  extensors  in  the  same  reflex-response.  Of  the 
two  reflexes  on  extensors  and  flexors  respectively,  either  the  one 
or  the  other  results,  but  not  the  two  together.  Thus,  in  my 
experience,  excitation  of  the  seventh  or  eighth  root  never  causes 
simultaneously  with  reflex  contraction  of  the  flexors  of  elbow  a 
contraction  of  that  part  of  the  triceps  which  extends  the  elbow. 
The  flexor-reflex  when  it  occurs  seems  therefore  to  exclude 
the  extensor-reflex,  and  vice  versa.  If  there  resulted  a  com- 
promise between  the  two  reflexes,  so  that  each  reflex  had  a 
share  in  the  resultant,  the  compound  would  be  an  action  which 
was  neither  the  appropriate  flexion  nor  the  appropriate  exten- 
sion. Were  there  to  occur  at  the  final  common  path  algebraical 
summation  of  the  influence  exerted  on  it  by  two  opposed  re- 
1  Baglioni's  results  288  support  this  inference. 


ii8  INTERACTION  BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

ceptive  arcs,  there  would  result  in  the  effector  organ  an  action 
adapted  to  neither  and  useless  for  the  purposes  of  either. 

In  the  Coelenterate,  Carmarina^  a  mechanical  stimulus  ap- 
plied to  the  subumbrella  causes,  as  in  another  Geryonid,  Tiar- 
opsis  indicansp  a  reflex  movement  that  brings  the  free  end 
of  the  manubrium  to  the  spot  touched.  Bethe  reports  ^63  that 
if  two  stimuli  are  applied  simultaneously  to  opposite  points  of 
the  discoid  subumbrella,  the  points  chosen  being  such  that  the 
manubrium  is  midway  between  them,  the  manubrium  is  moved 
toward  the  point  at  which  the  stimulus  applied  was  the  stronger. 
He  adds  that  if  both  stimuli  are  of  exactly  equal  strength  the 
manubrium  remains  unmoved  and  uncontracted.  To  obtain 
such  a  result  as  this  last  with  antagonistic  spinal  reflexes  in  the 
vertebrate  would  obviously  be  more  difficult,  because  the  more 
complex  the  preparation  and  the  nervous  system  involved,  the 
more  difficult  it  will  be  at  any  moment  to  exactly  balance  the 
two  reflexes.  But,  apart  from  that,  the  observation  on  Carmarina 
is  an  analogue  of  that  in  the  monkey's  arm.  £| 

This  dilemma  between  reflexes  would  seem  to  be  a  problem 
of  frequent  recurrence  in  reflex  co-ordination.  We  note  an 
orderly  sequence  of  actions  in  the  movement  of  animals,  even 
in  cases  where  every  observer  admits  that  the  co-ordination 
is  merely  reflex.  We  see  one  act  succeed  another  without  con- 
fusion. Yet,  tracing  this  sequence  to  its  external  causes,  we 
recognize  that  the  usual  thing  in  nature  is  not  for  one  exciting 
stimulus  to  begin  immediately  after  another  ceases,  but  for  an 
array  of  environmental  agents  acting  concurrently  on  the  animal 
at  any  moment  to  exhibit  correlative  change  in  regard  to  it,  so 
that  one  or  other  group  of  them  becomes  —  generally  by  in- 
crease in  intensity  —  temporarily  prepotent.  Thus  there  domi- 
nates now  this  group,  now  that  group  in  turn.  It  may  happen 
that  one  stimulus  ceases  coincidently  as  another  begins,  but  as  a 
rule  one  stimulus  overlaps  another  in  regard  to  time.  Thus  each 
reflex  breaks  in  upon  a  condition  of  relative  equilibrium,  which 
latter  is  itself  reflex.  In  the  simultaneous  correlation  of  reflexes 
some  reflexes  combine  harmoniously,  being  reactions  that  mu- 
tually reinforce.     These    may   be   termed   allied  reflexes,  and 


IV] 


PRINCIPLE    OF   THE   COMMON    PATH 


119 


the  neural  arcs  which  they  employ  allied  arcs.  On  the  other 
hand,  some  reflexes,  as  mentioned  above,  are  antagonistic  one  to 
another  and  incompatible.  These  do  not  mutually  reinforce, 
but  stand  to  each  other  in  inhibitory  relation.  One  of  them 
inhibits  the  other,  or  a  whole  group  of  others.  These  reflexes 
may  in  regard  to  one  another  be  termed  antagonistic ;  and  the 
reflex  or  group  of  reflexes  which  succeeds  in  inhibiting  its 
opponents  may  be  termed  "  prepotent "  for  the  time  being. 


Figure  38.  —  Summation  effect  {immediate  spinal  induction)  between  the  arcs  Ra  and  R3of 
Fig.  39  B.  FC  the  flexor  muscle  of  the  hip.  Sa  the  signal  line  marking  the  period  of 
stimulation  of  the  skin  belonging  to  arc  Ra  (Fig.  39  B)  of  the  shoulder  skin.  The  strength 
of  stimulus  is  arranged  to  be  subminimal,  so  that  a  reflex-response  in  fc  is  not  obtained. 
S^  the  signal  line  marking  the  period  of  stimulation,  also  subminimal  of  a  point  of  shoulder 
skin  8  centimeters  from  Ra.  Though  the  two  stimuli  applied  separately  are  each  unable  to 
evoke  the  reflex,  when  applied  contemporaneously  they  quickly  evoke  the  reflex.  The 
two  arcs  Ra  and  R/5,  therefore,  reinforce  each  other  in  their  action  on  the  final  common 
path  FC.    Time  in  fifths  of  seconds.    Read  from  left  to  right. 


120         INTERACTION  BETWEEN  REFLEXES      [Lect. 

Allied  reflexes.  The  action  of  the  principle  of  the  final  com- 
mon path  may  be  instanced  in  regard  to  "  allied  arcs  "  in  the 
scratch-reflex  as  follows.  If,  while  the  scratch-reflex  is  being 
elicited  from  a  skin  point  at  the  shoulder,  a  second  point 
distant,  e.  g.  lO  cent,  from  the  other  point  but  also  in  the  re- 
ceptive field  of  skin,  be  stimulated,  the  stimulation  at  this 
second  point  favours  the  reaction  from  the  first  point.  This  is 
well  seen  when  the  stimulus  at  each  point  is  of  subminimal  in- 
tensity. The  two  stimuli,  though  each  unable  separately  to 
invoke  the  reflex,  yet  do  so  when  applied  both  at  the  same  time 
(Fig.  38).  This  is  not  due  to  overlapping  spread  of  the  feeble 
currents  about  the  stigmatic  poles  of  the  two  circuits  used. 
Weak  cocainization  of  either  of  the  two  skin  points  annuls  it 
Moreover,  it  occurs  when  localized  mechanical  stimuli  are  used. 
It  therefore  seems  that  the  arcs  from  the  two  points,  e.  g.  Ra 
and  RyS  (Fig.  39  B)  have  such  a  mutual  relation  that  reaction 
of  one  of  them  reinforces  reaction  of  the  other,  as  judged  by  the 
effect  on  the  final  common  path. 

It  is  obvious  that  such  reinforcement  —  immediate  spinal  I 
induction  may  occur  in  either  of  two  ways.  The  diagram 
(Fig.  39  B)  treats  the  final  common  path  as  if  it  consisted  of 
a  single  individual  neurone.  The  single  neurone  of  the  dia- 
gram stands  for  several  thousands.  It  may  be  (i)  that  when 
the  reflex  is  excited  from  Ra  only  a  particular  group  of  the 
motor  neurones  composing  the  final  common  path  is  thrown 
into  action,  and  similarly  another  particular  group  when  the 
reflex  is  excited  from  R/S.  If  the  two  groups  in  the  final 
common  path  are  separate  groups,  the  explanation  of  the 
reinforcement  shown  in  the  muscular  response  may  be  by  me- 
chanical summation  of  contraction  occurring  in  two  separate 
fields  of  muscular  tissue,  the  contraction  of  each  too  slight  to 
cause  perceptible  movement  by  itself  without  the  other.  In 
other  words,  the  reinforcement  would  be  due  not  to  the  response 
in  the  set  of  neurones  comprising  the  final  common  path  (FC 
Fig.  39  B),  being  neurone  for  neurone  more  intense  under  the 
combined  stimulation  of  Ra  and  R/S  than  under  stimulation 
of  either  singly,  but  the  result  would  arise  from  the  number  of 


ALLIED   REFLEXES 


Figure  39.  —  A.  The  "receptive  field,"  as  revealed  after  low  cervical  transection,  a  saddle- 
shaped  area  of  dorsal  skin,  whence  the  scratch-reflex  of  the  left  hind  limb  can  be  evoked. 
Ir  marks  the  position  of  the  last  rib. 

B.  Diagram  of  the  spinal  arcs  involved.  L,  receptive  or  afferent  nerve-path  from 
the  left  foot;  r,  receptive  nerve-path  from  the  opposite  foot;  Ra,  r/3,  receptive  nerve- 
paths  from  hairs  in  the  dorsal  skin  of  the  left  side;  FC,  the  final  common  path,  in  this 
case  the  motor  neurone  to  a  flexor  muscle  of  the  hip ;  Pa,  p/3,  proprio-spinal  neurones. 


neurones  in  action  in  FC  being  simply  greater  under  the  stimu- 
lation of  the  two  skin  points  than  under  stimulation  of  one  of 
them  only. 

On  the  other  hand,  it  may  be  (2)  that  all  the  neurones  com- 
posing the  final  common  path  constitute  together  one  almost 
unitary  apparatus,  so    that   stimulation  at   Ka  excites  or  can 


122        INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

excite  them  all,  and  similarly  stimulation  at  RyS  excites  or 
can  excite  them  all.  The  question,  therefore,  regarding  the 
mode  of  the  reinforcement  is  a  question  between  intensity  and 
extensity.  The  scratch-reflex  affords  some  opportunity  for  ex- 
amining this  question.  The  rhythm  of  the  reflex  has  practically 
the  same  frequency  whether  the  reflex  be  excited  strongly  or 
feebly :  thus,  whether  the  amplitude  of  the  contractions  be  great 
or  small,  they  recur  with  practically  the  same  frequence.  Sup- 
pose the  reflex  be  excited  by  stimulation  of  the  skin  point  R  a 
(Fig-  39  B),  and  suppose  the  stimulus  is  weak,  producing  only  a 
feeble  reflex.  Then  let  another  skin  point  R^  (Fig.  39 B)  be 
stimulated  while  Ra  is  being  stimulated,  and  let  the  stimuli  at 
RyS  be  timed  so  as  to  fall  alternately  with  those  applied  at  Ra. 
Then  if  the  two  paths  impinge  on  two  different  sets  of  units  in 
the  compound  group  of  motor  neurones  composing  the  final 
common  path,  evidence  of  two  rhythms  should  appear,  for  the 
muscle-fibres  (of  the  flexors  of  the  hip)  can  respond  to  a  much 
quicker  rhythm  than  four  per  second.  But,  in  fact,  the  result  is 
that  the  rhythm  appears  unquickened  and  unaltered  (Figs.  18, 
19,  20,  21).  There  is  not  even  a  break  or  interference  in  it. 
It  might  be  thought,  therefore,  that  for  some  reason  the  stimu- 
lation of  the  second  point,  R/S,  is  remaining  ineffective  alto- 
gether. But  that  is  not  so,  because  the  stimulation  at  R/S  has 
often  the  effect  of  increasing  the  amplitude  (Fig.  18)  of  the 
individual  beats  of  the  rhythmic  reflex,  though  it  does  not  alter 
the  rhythm.  This  change  in  amplitude  proves  that  the  reflex  is 
also  in  action  from  the  second  skin  point  as  well  as  from  the  first. 
But  there  is  no  interference  of  the  rhythms  of  the  two  reflexes. 
Evidently  the  central  mechanism  on  which  R/^  acts  is  subjected 
by  Ra  to  a  refractory  state  which  the  stimulation  at  R/3  does 
not  break  through.  That  is,  the  refractory  state  obtaining  in 
the  central  mechanism  under  action  from  Ra  obtains  at  the 
same  moment  for  excitation  reaching  it  from  RyS.  The  central 
mechanism  acted  on  by  Kff  must  therefore  belong  in  common 
to  the  reflexes  from  Ra  and  Ry8  respectively.  And  since  the 
experiment  can  be  repeated  with  a  great  number  of  different 
pairs  of  points  in  the  receptive  field,  practically  the  whole  of  the 


IV]  IMMEDIATE   SPINAL   INDUCTION  123 

neurones  of  FC  are  common  to  all  the  receptive  points  in  the 
receptive  field.  Similarly  it  is  shown  by  Zwaardemaker  ^^2  that 
the  refractory  phase  demonstrated  by  him  in  reflex  deglutition 
spreads  to  the  whole  of  the  reflex  centre,  both  right  and  left. 

Again,  it  was  shown  above,  under  the  heading  of  summation, 
that  although  a  single-induction  shock,  even  though  strong,  does 
not  in  my  experience  ever  evoke  a  scratch-reflex,  a  series  of 
even  feeble  shocks  does  so  by  summation.  But  in  order  to  act 
by  summation  the  individual  shocks  must  follow  each  other  at 
not  too  long  an  interval  of  time,  the  interval  being  caeteris  pari- 
bus shorter  the  less  intense  the  shocks.  Suppose  an  induction 
shock  be  applied  to  Ra  at  such  a  frequence,  e,  g.  once  a  second, 
that  at  the  intensity  chosen  they  fail  to  evoke  the  reflex.  Sup- 
pose that  a  series  of  induction  shocks  be  applied  to  R/S  similarly 
unable  to  evoke  the  reflex.  Then  suppose  that  while  the 
stimuli  are  being  applied  to  Ra  and  fail  to  evoke  the  reflex, 
the  other  series  of  stimuli  are  applied  to  R^S,  and  are  so  ap- 
plied that  each  stimulus  at  RyS  falls  at  a  moment  of  time  mid- 
way between  the  moments  of  application  of  the  stimuli  at  Ra. 
The  stimuli  thus  conjoined  suffice  to  evoke  the  reflex.  Evi- 
dently the  internal  excitatory  change  is  not  confined  to  the  arcs 
to  whose  receptive  ends  the  external  stimulus  is  actually  applied. 
It  spreads  to  other  arcs  belonging  to  the  same  "  type-reflex," 
especially  to  those  arising  near  to  those  actually  stimulated  in 
the  receptive  field.  A  subminimal  stimulus  at  one  point  in  the 
field  favours  response  to  a  subsequent  stimulus  at  a  second  point 
in  the  field  even  8  centimeters  distant  —  so  long  as  the  second 
stimulus  follows  within  summation  time;  but  the  summation 
time  is  shorter  than  when  stimuli  follow  each  other  at  one  and 
the  same  spot,  and  is  especially  so  when  the  points  stimulated 
lie  distant  from  one  another.  Hence  we  may  draw  some  sort 
of  picture  of  the  extent  of  the  excitatory  internal  change  in- 
duced in  this  reflex  mechanism  by  a  single  momentary  stimu- 
lus: as  to  distribution  in  time  the  change  fades  off"  gradually 
from  an  early  maximum  to  a  trace  just  detectable  after  14000-, 
if  the  stimulus  be  strong :  as  to  distribution  in  space  it  spreads 
from  the  peripherally  stimulated  arcs  A  A  themselves  as  centre 


124         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

to  the  intraspinal  parts  of  other  arcs  of  the  same  type-reflex,  but 
among  these  it  affects  those  starting  in  the  skin  as  neighbours 
to  A  A  more  than  ones  more  distant  in  origin,  and  it  endures 
less  long  in  these  than  in  its  own  arcs ;  hence  the  shorter  sum- 
mation time.  Exner  early  insisted  on  the  close  connection 
between  "  facilitation  "  (bahnung)  and  summation.  The  above 
immediate  spinal  induction  illustrates  it  well. 

The  mutual  reinforcement  of  action  exercised  by  the  two 
scratch-reflexes  one  upon  another  appears  therefore  to  be  an 
affair  of  intensity.  This  does  not,  however,  exclude  the  exist- 
ence of  extensity  as  a  factor  also  in  some  degree.  There  is 
evidence,  adverted  to  above  (p.  ^6^  Lect.  Ill),  that  makes  it  likely 
that  in  very  weak  reflexes  not  all  the  individual  neurones 
composing  the  final  common  path  are  in  action,  although  in 
stronger  reflex  reactions  all  may  be  in  action. 

In  the  scratch-reflex  the  mutual  reinforcing  power  between 
the  reflexes  falls  as  the  distance  between  the  receptors  of  the 
arcs  increases.  The  nearer  the  skin  points  of  Ra  and  R/3  lie 
together  the  greater  the  mutual  reinforcement  between  the  action 
of  their  arcs  on  FC.  This  suggests  an  explanation  by  physical 
diff'usion  of  the  stimulating  currents  applied  to  Ra  and  R/3 ;  but 
for  the  reasons  above  mentioned  this  overlap  of  stimulus  can,  I 
consider,  be  excluded.  Light  is  however  thrown  on  this  propor- 
tion between  the  degree  of  reinforcement  and  the  degree  of 
nearness  of  the  receptive  points  by  another  feature  of  the  re- 
flex. The  scratch-reflex  in  the  spinal  dog  carries  the  foot  ap- 
proximately toward  the  place  of  stimulation.  In  the  spinal  dog 
the  reflex  does  not  succeed  in  bringing  the  foot  actually  to  the 
irritated  skin  point,  yet  when  the  irritation  lies  far  forward 
the  foot  is  carried  further  forward,  and  when  the  irritation  is 
far  back  the  foot  is  carried  further  back.  A  scratch-reflex 
evoked  by  a  stimulus  applied  far  back  and  high  up  in  the 
dorsal  skin  is  therefore  not  wholly  like  a  scratch-reflex  evoked 
from  far  forward  and  low  down.  These  diff*erences  are  easily 
registered  in  graphic  tracings  of  the  movement  at  hip  (Fig. 
40).  It  is  found  that  the  greater  the  likeness  between  the  two 
scratch-reflexes   which   two  separate   skin   points   initiate,   the 


r 


ALLIED   REFLEXES 


125 


Figure  40.  —  Tracing  of  the  hip  flexion  in  the  '*  scratch-reflex."  The  reflex  was  elicited  by- 
unipolar  faradization  of  a  point  of  skin  rather  far  back  and  near  the  dorsum  in  the  recep- 
tive field.  Considerable  tonic  flexion  is  seen  to  accompany  the  clonic  scratching  move- 
ments. Time  in  fifths  of  seconds.  The  lower  signal  marks  the  period  of  stimulation. 
Compare  figures  14,  18,  19,  in  which  the  skin  points  excited  lay  farther  forward  and  more 
ventral  in  the  field  and  the  subtratum  of  steady  flexion  is  much  less. 


126         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

stronger  the  mutual  reinforcement  between  the  action  of  those 
two  receptive  points  upon  the  final  common  path  FC  (Figs.  38  and 
41).  In  other  words,  the  coalition  between  reflexes  is  greater  the 
greater  the  likeness  between  them,  and  that  likeness  increases 
with  the  nearness  of  their  receptive  points  to  one  another  in 
the  skin  surface.  I  have  seen  the  mutual  reinforcement  demon- 
strable with  skin  points  20  centimeters  apart  in  the  receptive 
field  of  the  scratch-reflex,  but  I  have  failed  to  find  this  mutual 
reinforcement  between  the  most  distant  arcs  of  the  receptive 
field.  Whether  coalition  fades  into  mere  indifference  or  passes 
over  into  antagonism  I  have  not  at  present  the  evidence  to 
judge. 

The  whole  collection  of  points  of  skin  surface  from  which 
the  scratch-reflex  can  be  elicited  may  conveniently  be  termed 
the  receptive  field  of  that  reflex.  And  the  receptive  field  of  a 
reflex  is  analyzable  into  points  from  each  of  which  the  reflex 
can  be  evoked.     But  the  reflex  as  elicited  from  various  points 


Figure  41.  — Similar  to  Fig.  38,  but  with  greater  separation  of  the  two  skin  points  stimu- 
lated :  in  A  the  separation  of  the  two  points  was  15  centimeters,  in  B  it  was  20  centi- 
meters. 


IV]    TYPE-REFLEXES  AND  ALLIED  REFLEXES     127 

in  its  receptive  field  is  not  in  the  case  of  all  the  points  exactly 
the  same  reflex;  e.  g.,  the  foot  is  directed  to  somewhat  different 
places  according  as  the  scratch-reflex  is  elicited  from  this  or  that 
point.  A  similar  feature  is  seen  in  the  "  wisch-reflex  "  of  the 
spinal  frog's  hind  leg.  That  is  to  say,  when  we  speak  of  the 
"  scratch-reflex  "  in  general,  what  we  mean  strictly  speaking  is  a 
group  of  reflexes  all  more  or  less  alike,  all  using  approximately  the 
same  motor  apparatus  in  approximately  the  same  way,  and  all 
more  or  less  conforming  to  the  same  type.  And  this  group  of  indi- 
vidual reflexes  forms  a  physiological  group  not  only  on  account 
of  their  similarity,  but  also  because  they  act  harmoniously  upon 
the  same  final  common  path,  and  in  many  cases  reinforcement 
occurs  between  them  in  their  action  on  that  common  path. 
Their  intraspinal  mechanisms  are  more  or  less  knit  together 
into  an  harmonious  whole.  A  reflex,  e.  g.  the  scratch-reflex, 
when  referred  to  in  general,  may  be  conveniently  termed  a 
type-reflex.  The  kind  of  harmonious  relationship  which  holds 
between  the  individual  reflexes  comprised  under  one  and  the 
same  type-reflex  may  be  indicated  by  recognizing  them  as  **  al- 
lied reflexes  "  and  their  arcs  as  "  allied  arcs." 

Similarly  with  the  various  other  reflexes.  The  flexion-reflex 
of  the  hind  limb,  the  pinna-reflex,  the  extensor-thrust,  the 
crossed  extension-reflex  of  the  hind  limb,  the  torticollis  reflex, 
etc. ;  these  are  each  of  them  type-reflexes.  Each  is  a  group  of 
reflexes.  The  individual  reflexes  comprised  in  each  of  these 
type-reflexes  have  such  mutual  relationship  between  themselves 
that  they  act  harmoniously  together  on  the  same  final  common 
path,  and  are  therefore  "  allied  reflexes "  and  employ  "  allied 
arcs." 

The  extent  of  the  receptive  field  of  each  type-reflex  is  usually 
wide.  It  is  much  wider  in  some  type-reflexes  than  in  others ; 
thus,  that  of  the  direct  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  of  the 
dog  is  more  extensive  than  that  of  the  extensor-thrust  of  the 
limb.  Within  the  receptive  field  of  any  given  type-reflex  not  all 
the  receptive  points  equally  potently  excite  the  reflex.  From 
certain  areas  of  points  the  reflex  can  be  most  easily  evoked,  from 
certain  others  least  easily,  and  from  the  rest  of  the  field  with  in- 


128         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

termediate  degrees  of  facility.  The  area  whence  the  reflex  can 
be  evoked  with  most  difficulty  is  usually  the  circumferential  zone 
of  the  field,  the  width  of  the  zone  varying  along  different  radii. 
The  area  where  the  threshold  stimulus  is  lowest  lies  usually  fairly 
remote,  though  not  equally  remote,  from  all  the  borders  of  the 
field.  The  reflex  effect  of  a  weak  stimulus  in  this  central  focal 
area  seems  to  resemble  the  effect  of  a  stronger  stimulus  applied 
in  the  border  zone  of  the  field.  Reflexes  of  an  intensity  unob- 
tainable from  the  border  zone  of  the  field  can  be  easily  provoked 
by  stimulation  of  the  focal  area  of  the  field.  In  the  flexion-reflex 
of  the  dog's  hind  limb  the  toe-pads  and  plantar  cushion  are  in 
the  focal  area  of  the  receptive  field.  In  the  scratch-reflex  of  the 
dog  the  focal  area  is  along  that  part  of  the  field  that  lies  next  to 
the  mid-dorsal  line  of  the  trunk,  and  especially  (as  seen  after 
low  cervical  transection)  near  the  posterior  end  of  the  scapular 
region;  e.g,  in  Fig.  39  A,  from  5  to  15  in  the  horizontal  figures 
and  dorsal  to  9  in  the  vertical  row.  The  difference  between  the 
threshold  value  of  the  stimulus  for  the  reflex  at  different  points 
in  the  field  is  very  considerable  indeed.  Although  the  absolute 
value  of  the  threshold  may  vary  considerably  in  one  and  the 
same  animal  at  different  times,  even  from  day  to  day,  the  rela- 
tive values  as  between  separate  areas  in  the  same  field  is 
usually  about  the  same.  But  this  relative  value  may  be  upset  by 
"  local  fatigue,"  etc.  The  coalescence  of  allied  reflexes  excited 
from  one  receptive  field  tends  to  make  weak  stimuli  applied  to  an 
extensive  area  equivalent  to  intenser  stimuli  applied  to  a  smaller 
area.  G.  H.  Parker  ^^  shows  that  in  the  positive  phototropism  of 
the  frog  to  light  falling  on  its  skin  the  strength  of  the  reaction 
varies  in  proportion  with  the  extent  of  skin  exposed  to  the  light. 
^  Reflex  complication.  One  and  the  same  field  of  receptive 
surface  may,  and  usually  does,  contain  receptive  points  of  more 
than  a  single  species.  Thus,  a  skin-field  may  contain  receptors 
some  of  which  are  adapted  for  mechanical  stimuli,  some  for 
chemical,  some  for  thermal,  and  so  on.  In  this  case  receptors 
of  two  different  species  may  not  both  of  them  initiate  reflexes 
which  belong  to  the  same  type-reflex,  i.  e.  which  have  the  rela- 
tion to  one  another  of  "  allied  reflexes."     For  instance,  in  the 


IV]  RECEPTIVE    FIELD    OF   A   REFLEX  129 

planta  of  the  dog's  foot  receptors  coexist  ^^^  of  which  one  set 
are  excited  by  mechanical  stimuli  of  harmless  (tactual)  kind,  the 
other  set  by  stimuli  of  nocuous  kind.  The  reflexes  elicited  from 
the  limb  through  these  two  kinds  of  receptors  respectively 
do  not  reinforce  each  other  but  oppose  each  other.  On  the 
other  hand,  in  the  tentacles  of  the  Actinian,  Aiptasis  saxicola, 
there  coexist  at  the  surface  receptors  of  two  species,^^^  one  re- 
ceptive for  tactual  stimuli  the  other  for  certain  chemical  stimuli 
(Nagel).  The  reflexes  elicited  through  these  by  combination 
of  mechanical  with  certain  chemical  stimuli  seem  to  combine 
harmoniously  and  mutually  reinforce  each  other  (Nagel).  And 
a  similar  occurrence  seems  evidenced  by  observations  on  the 
barblets  of  Siluroid  fishes,  e.g.  Ameiurus^^  (C  J.  Herrick). 
The  combining  of  such  reflexes  is  comparable  with  the  associa- 
tive combination  of  disparate  sensations  for  which  Herbart^^ 
introduced  the  term  "  complication." 

Analogy  exists  here,  as  it  should,  between  the  compatibility 
of  reflex  movements  from  two  receptors  of  different  species  and 
the  compatibility  of  sensations  which,  judging  by  inference  from 
our  own  introspection,  might  be  initiated  from  such  receptors. 
Skin-pain  is  sensually  incompatible  with  pure  touch,  the  dolor- 
ous suppressing  the  tactual,  just  as  the  noci-ceptive  reflex  in  the 
"  spinal "  dog's  hind  leg  suppresses  the  merely  tango-ceptive. 
But  gustatory  and  tactual  sensations  excited  from  the  same  re- 
ceptive surface,  e.  g.  the  tongue,  habitually  blend  harmoniously. 

Proprio-tcptive  reflexea.  There  exists  a  further  important  class 
of  cases  mvWhich  reflexes  have  "  allied  "  relation.  Throughout 
a  vast  range  of  animal  types  the  bulk  formed  by  the  organism 
presents  to  the  environment  a  surface  sheet  of  cells,  and,  beneath 
that,  a  mass  of  cells  more  or  less  screened  from  the  environment 
by  the  surface  sheet  Many  of  the  agencies  by  which  the  envi- 
ronment acts  6n  the  organism  do  not  penetrate  it  far  enough  to 
reach  the  cells  of  the  deep  mass  inside.  Bedded  in  the  surface 
layer  of  the  organism  are  numbers  of  receptor  cells  constituted 
in  adaptation  to  the  stimuli  delivered  by  environmental  agencies. 
But  the  organism  itself,  like  the  world  surrounding  it,  is  a  field 
of  ceaseless  change,  where  internal  energy  is  continually  being 

9 


I30        INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

liberated,  whence  chemical,  thermal,  mechanical,  and  electrical 
effects  appear.  It  is  a  microcosm  in  which  forces  which  can  act 
as  stimuli  are  at  work  as  in  the  macrocosm  around.  The  deep 
tissues  underlying  the  surface  sheet  are  not  provided  with 
receptors  of  the  same  kinds  as  those  of  the  surface,  yet  they 
are  not  devoid  of  receptors.  They  have  receptors  specific  to 
themselves.  The  receptors  which  lie  in  the  depth  of  the  organism 
are  adapted  for  excitation  consonantly  with  changes  going  on  in 
the  organism  itself,  particularly  in  its  muscles  and  their  acces- 
sory organs  (tendons,  joints,  blood-vessels,  etc).  Since  in  this 
field  the  stimuli  to  the  receptors  are  given  by  the  organism  itself, 
their  field  may  be  called  the  proprio-ceptive  field. 

There  exist,  therefore,  two  primary  distributions  of  the  re- 
ceptor organs,  each  a  field  in  certain  respects  fundamentally 
different  from  the  other.  The  surface  field  lies  freely  open  to 
the  numberless  vicissitudes  of  the  environment.  It  has  felt  for 
countless  ages  the  full  stream  of  the  varied  agencies  forever 
pouring  upon  it  from  the  outside  world.  This  field,  extero-ceptive 
as  it  may  be  called,  is  rich  in  the  number  and  variety  of  re- 
ceptors which  adaptation  has  evolved  in  it. 

The  excitation  of  the  receptors  of  the  proprio-ceptive  field 
in  contradistinction  from  those  of  the  exteroceptive  is  related 
only  secondarily  to  the  agencies  of  the  environment.  The 
proprio-ceptive  receive  their  stimulation  by  some  action,  e.  g. 
a  muscular  contraction,  which  was  itself  a  primary  reaction  to 
excitation  of  a  surface  receptor  by  the  environment.  The  pri- 
mary reaction  is  excited  in  the  majority  of  cases  byra  receptor 
of  the  extero-ceptive  field,  that  field  so  rich  in  the  number  and 
the  variety  of  its  receptors.  Reflexes  arising  from  proprio- 
ceptive organs  come  therefore  to  be  habitually  attached  and 
appended  to  certain  reflexes  excited  by  extero-ceptive  organs. 
The  reaction  of  the  animal  to  stimulation  of  one  of  its  extero- 
ceptors  excites  certain  tissues,  and  the  activity  thus  produced  in 
these  latter  tissues  excites  in  them  their  receptors,  which  are 
propriO'Ceptors.  Thus,  in  a  muscular  movement  induced  by  a 
stimulus  to  the  skin  of  the  spinal  dog,  the  change  in  form  and 
tension  of  the  muscles,  the  movements  of  the  joints,  etc.,  excite 


IVJ     PROPRIO-CEPTIVE  AND  EXTERO-CEPTIVE     131 

the  receptors  in  these  structures,  and  these  in  turn  initiate  a 
reflex  in  their  own  arcs  and  their  reaction  often  has  an  "  allied  '* 
relation  to  the  reflex  reaction  excited  from  the  skin. 

Alliance  of  proprio-ceptive  "with  extero-ceptive  reflexes.  In 
one  of  the  type-reflexes  previously  described,  namely  the  scratch- 
reflex,  the  reflex-arcs  which  provoke  the  reflex  arise  in  a 
large  continuous  area  of  skin,  and  all  excite  the  same  motor 
neurones,  that  is,  are  mutually  related  as  allied  arcs.  The 
area  of  skin  whence  these  arcs  arise  we  termed  the  recep- 
tive field  of  the  reflex.  The  afferent  nerves  of  the  muscles 
which  execute  the  scratching  movement  do  not,  when  them- 
selves excited,  evoke  the  scratch-reflex;  nor  does  the  sever- 
ance of  the  afferent  nerves  of  the  muscles  obviously  impair 
or  alter  the  scratch-reflex.  With  the  flexion-reflex  of  the  limb 
it  is  different.  The  reflex,  like  the  scratch-reflex,  has  a  cutane- 
ous field  of  origin.  It  is  provocable  from  arcs  arising  in  a 
large  area  of  the  skin  covering  the  hind  limb.  But  the  flexion- \ 
reflex  can  in  addition  be  excited  from  various  of  the  afferent" 
nerves  of  the  muscles  of  the  limb.  Thus  stimulation  of  the 
central  end  of  the  nerve  of  the  flexor  muscles  themselves  excites 
the  reflex.  It  is  similarly  elicitable  from  the  afferent  nerve  of 
the  extensor  muscle  (vasto  crureus)  of  the  knee.  And  the 
reflex  excited  from  the  muscles  of  the  limb  allies  itself  with 
the  reflex  excited  from  the  skin  of  the  limb.  A  subliminal 
stimulation  of  the  afferent  nerve  of  the  hamstring  muscles 
applied  simultaneously  with  a  subliminal  stimulation  of  the 
skin  of  the  foot  results  in  a  marked  flexion-reflex. 

In  the  case  of  the  flexion-reflex,  therefore,  the  receptive  field 
includes  not  only  reflex-arcs  arising  in  the  surface  field,  but 
reflex-arcs  arising  in  the  depth  of  the  limb.  Combined  there- 
fore with  an  extero-ceptive  area,  this  reflex  has,  included  in  its 
receptive  field,  2.  proprio-ceptive  field.  The  reflex-arcs  belonging 
to  its  extero-ceptive  and  proprio-ceptive  components  co-operate 
harmoniously  together,  and  mutually  reinforce  each  other's 
action.  In  this  class  of  cases  the  reflex  from  the  muscle- 
joint  apparatus  seems  to  reiiiforce  the  reflex  initiated  from  the 
skin. 


132         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

Reflex  flexion  of  the  leg  is  induced  by  stimulation  of  the 
central  end  of  the  nerve  of  a  hamstring  muscle.  Since  me- 
chanical stimulation  of  these  flexor  muscles,  e.  g.  kneading  or 
squeezing  them,  excites  a  reflex  inhibition  of  the  contraction  of 
their  antagonists,  which  as  we  have  seen  is  part  of  the  flexion- 
reflex  itself,  it  would  seem  likely  that  their  own  contraction  will 
excite  a  flexion-reflex.  A  flexion-reflex  excited  from  the  skin 
would  thus  in  its  progress  tend  to  induce  a  secondary  flexion- 
reflex  which  would  reinforce  the  primary  one,  for  when  excited 
apart  the  reflexes  excited  from  an  aff"erent  nerve  of  the  foot  and 
from  the  hamstring  nerve  are  closely  similar  (Fig.  37).  The 
case  therefore  resembles  that  of  the  reflexes  from  two  adjacent 
spots  in  the  receptive  field  of  the  scratch-reflex.  The  reflex 
elicited  from  the  skin  of  the  foot  and  that  elicited  from  the 
hamstring  muscle  are  "  allied  "  reflexes.  There  is  here  alliance 
and  "bahnung"  between  a  reflex  of  the  proprio-ceptive  field  and 
a  reflex  of  the  extero-ceptive  field. 

Similarly,  if  the  knee-jerk  is  accepted  as  a  sign  of  a  tonic 
reflex  originated  by  the  afl'erent  nerve-endings  in  the  knee-jerk 
muscles  themselves,  many  reflexes  elicitable  from  the  extero- 
ceptive surface  are  well  known  to  reinforce  it.  A  comprehensive 
account  of  these  was  furnished  in  Sternberg's  monograph  ^^ 
(1893).  Here  again  the  reflexes  which  are  "allied,"  exhibiting 
reinforcement  and  "bahnung,"  belong  not  in  the  ordinary  sense 
to  the  same  category,  but  have  reflex-arcs  commencing  in 
receptive  organs  of  different  species.  Yet  the  arcs  are  "  allied 
arcs,"  for  they  act  harmoniously  on  the  same  final  common  path. 

That  the  prolongation  of  the  reflex  contractions  character- 
istic of  strychnine  is  due  to  excitation  of  muscular  (proprio- 
ceptive) reflexes  (Baglioni)  ^^'''  ^^^  secondary  to  a  reflex  elicited 
from  other  receptors  is  again  a  further  illustration  of  the 
secondary  relation  of  proprio-ceptive  reflexes  to  extero-ceptive 
pointed  out  above. 

"Wider  combinations  of  reflexes.  And  reflexes  whose  arcs 
commence  in  receptive  fields  even  wider  apart  than  those  men- 
tioned above  may  also  have  '*  allied  "  relation.  In  the  bulbo- 
spinal dog  stimulation  of  the  outer  digit  of  the  hind  foot  will 


IV]       COMBINATIONS  OF  ALLIED  REFLEXES        133 

evoke  reflex  flexion  of  the  leg,  and  stimulation  of  each  of  the 
other  digits  evokes  practically  the  same  reflex ;  and  if  stimula- 
tion of  several  of  these  points  be  simultaneously  combined  the 
same  reflex  as  a  result  is  obtained  more  readily  than  if  one 
only  of  these  points  is  stimulated.  And  to  these  stimulations 
may  be  added  simultaneously  stimulation  of  points  in  the 
crossed  fore  foot;  stimulation  there  yields  by  itself  flexion  01 
the  hind  leg;  and  under  the  simultaneous  stimulation  of  fore 
and  hind  foot  the  flexion  of  the  leg  goes  on  as  before,  though 
perhaps  more  readily;  that  is,  the  several  individual  reflexes 
harmonize  in  their  effect  on  the  hind  limb.  Further,  to  these 
may  be  added  simultaneous  stimulation  of  the  tail,  and  of  the 
crossed  pinna ;  and  the  reflexes  of  these  stimulations  all  coa- 
lesce in  the  same  way  in  flexion  of  the  hind  leg.  Exner^^ 
has  shown  that  in  exciting  different  points  of  the  central  ner- 
vous system  itself,  points  widely  apart  exert  bahnung  for  one 
another's  reactions,  and  for  various  reflex  reactions  induced 
from  the  skin.  Thus  reflexes  originated  at  diff'erent  distant 
points,  and  passing  through  paths  widely  separate  in  the  brain, 
converge  to  the  same  motor  mechanism  (final  common  path) 
and  act  harmoniously  upon  it.  Reflex-arcs  from  widely  differ- 
ent parts  conjoin  and  pour  their  influence  harmoniously  into 
the  same  muscle.  The  motor  neurones  of  a  muscle  of  the 
knee  are  the  terminus  ad  qiiem  of  reflex-arcs  arising  in  re- 
ceptors not  only  of  its  own  foot,  but  from  the  crossed  fore 
foot  and  pinna,  and  tail,  also  undoubtedly  from  the  otic  laby- 
rinth, olfactory  organs,  and  eyes.  Thus,  if  we  take  as  a  stand- 
point any  motor-nerve  to  a  muscle  it  consists  of  a  number  of 
motor  neurones  which  are  more  or  less  bound  into  a  unit 
mechanism ;  among  the  reflex-actions  of  the  organism  a  number 
can  all  be  brought  together  as  a  groups  because  they  all  in 
their  course  converge  together  upon  this  motor  mechanism, 
this  final  common  paihy  activate  it,  and  are  in  harmonious 
mutual  relation  with  regard  to  it.  They  are  in  regard  to  it 
what  were  termed  above  "  allied  "  reflexes. 

Allied  inhibitory  reflexes.     The  examples  of  allied   reflexes 
cited  so  far  have  had  for  their  result  on  the  final  common  path 


134         INTERACTION  BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

an  increase  of  its  activity ;  that  is  to  say,  of  its  activity  as  a  dis- 
charger of  nervous  impulses.  But  the  same  final  common  path 
can  be  shown  to  be  connected  also  with  certain  reflexes  initi- 
able  from  other  receptive  points  which  depress  its  activity  as 
a  discharger  of  nervous  impulses.  The  reflexes  exerting  this 
influence  are  "  inhibitory,"  whereas  the  reflexes  mentioned  be- 
fore may  be  termed  "  excitatory."  Inhibitory  reflexes  are 
accessible  to  study  chiefly  through  the  kind  of  refractory  state 
which  they  impress  upon  the  commencement  of  the  efferent 
part  of  their  arc,  as  tested  by  concurrent  excitations  of  reflexes 
which  should  excite  it. 

Just  as  in  regard  to  one  and  the  same  final  common  path 
certain  excitatory  reflexes  act  harmoniously  together  and  rein- 
force one  another,  so  also  do  certain  inhibitory  reflexes.  Thus, 
reflex  inhibition  of  the  flexors  of  the  knee  (spinal  dog)  is 
regularly  excitable  by  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  a  digit  of  the 
crossed  hind  foot;  and  the  concurrent  stimulation  of  two  or 
more  digits  and  of  the  dorsum  pedis  of  the  crossed  foot  mutu- 
ally combine  and  reinforce  in  their  reflex  inhibition  of  the 
knee-flexor:  and  to  these  may  be  added  stimulation  of  the 
homonymous  fore  foot :  all  these  reflexes  combine  harmoniously 
together  in  exerting  a  conjoint  inhibitory  influence  on  the  knee- 
flexors.  The  alliance  between  reflexes  in  regard  to  any  one 
final  common  path  may  be  as  wide  and  strong  when  the  end- 
result  of  those  reflexes  is  in  the  form  of  inhibition  as  when  it  is 
in  the  form  of  excitation.  In  addition,  therefore,  to  the  category 
of  "  allied  excitatory "  reflexes  above  mentioned  there  is  a 
category  of  "allied  inhibitory"  reflexes.  Under  this  latter 
category  come  subgroups  analogous  to  the  four  already  men- 
tioned under  allied  excitatory  reflexes.  Thus :  the  reflex  from 
the  proprio-ceptive  nerves  of  the  hamstring  muscles  combines 
with  and  reinforces  the  flexion-reflex  from  the  skin  of  the  foot 
of  the  same  leg  in  a  resultant  reflex  inhibition  of  the  extensors 
of  the  homonymous  knee. 

But  there  are,  as  we  have  seen,  reflexes  which  are  neither 
purely  excitatory  nor  purely  inhibitory.  For  instance,  the 
flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  leg  (cat  and  dog)  is,  as  we  have  seen, 


IV]  ANTAGONISTIC   REFLEXES  135 

at  one  and  the  same  time  excitatory  of  the  flexor  neurones 
(knee)  and  inhibitory  of  the  extensor  neurones  (knee). 

These  reflexes  of  simultaneous  double-sign  may  have  "  allied  '* 
relation  with  one  another,  e.  g.y  the  individual  reflexes  of  the 
flexion  type-reflex. 

Also  there  are  other  reflexes  neither  purely  excitatory  nor 
purely  inhibitory,  namely,  the  reflexes  which  during  the  con- 
tinuance or  repetition  of  the  exciting  stimulus  exhibit  refractory 
period.  Several  rhythmic  reflexes  seem  of  this  character,  e,  g. 
the  swallowing  reflex,  the  scratch-reflex.  If  we  regard  refrac- 
tory phase  as  a  kind  of  inhibition,  then  these  reflexes  are,  as 
we  have  seen,  reflexes  of  successive  double-sign.  And  these 
also  can  be  "  aUied"  in  their  relation  one  to  another. 

Antagonistic  reflexes.  But  not  all  reflexes  connected  to  one 
and  the  same  final  common  path  stand  to  one  another  in  the 
relation  of  "  allied  reflexes."  Suppose  during  the  scratch-reflex 
a  stimulus  be  applied  to  the  foot  not  of  the  scratching  side  but 
of  the  opposite  side  (Fig.  39  B,  R).  The  left  leg,  which  is  ex- 
ecuting the  scratch-reflex  in  response  to  stimulation  of  the  left 
shoulder  skin  is  cut  short  in  its  movement  by  the  stimulation  of 
the  right  foot,  although  the  stimulus  at  the  shoulder  to  provoke 
the  scratch  movement  is  maintained  unaltered  all  the  time.  The 
stimulus  to  the  right  foot  will  temporarily  interrupt  a  scratch- 
reflex,  or  will  cut  it  short  or  will  delay  its  onset ;  which  it  does 
of  these  depends  on  the  time-relations  of  the  stimuli  (Fig.  42). 
The  inhibition  of  the  scratch-reflex  occurs  sometimes  when  the 
contraction  of  the  muscles  innervated  by  the  reflex  conflicting 
with  it  is  very  slight.  There  is  interference  between  the  two 
reflexes  and  the  one  is  inhibited  by  the  other.  The  final  com- 
mon path  used  by  the  left  scratch-reflex  is  also  common  to  the 
reflex  elicitable  from  the  right  foot.  This  latter  reflex  evokes 
at  the  opposite  (left)  knee  extension ;  in  doing  this  it  causes 
steady  excitation  of  extensor  neurones  of  that  knee  and  steadily 
inhibits  the  flexor  neurones.^^  But  the  scratch-reflex  causes 
rhythmic  excitation  of  the  flexor  neurones.  Therefore  these 
flexor  neurones  in  this  conflict  lie  as  a  final  common  path  under 


136  INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

the  influence  of  two  antagonistic  reflexes,  one  of  which  would 
excite  them  to  rhythmical  discharge  four  times  a  second,  while 
the  other  would  continuously  repress  all  discharge  in  them. 
There  is  here  an  antagonistic  relation  between  reflexes  em- 
bouching  on  one  and  the  same  final  common  path. 

In  all  these  forms  of  interference  there  is  a  competition,  as 
it  were,  between  the  excitatory  stimulus  used  for  the  one  reflex 
and  the  excitatory  stimulus  for  the  other.  Both  stimuli  are  in 
progress  together,  and  the  one  in  taking  eff"ect  precludes  the 
other's  taking  effect  as  far  as  the  final  common  path  is  con- 
cerned; and  the  precise  form  in  which  that  occurs  depends 
greatly  on  the  time-relations  of  application  of  the  two  stimuli 
competing  against  each  other. 

Again,  if,  while  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  shoulder  is 
evoking  the  scratch-reflex,  the  skin  of  the  hind  foot  of  the  same 
side  is  stimulated,  the  scratching  may  be  arrested  ^^  (F'g-  43)- 
Stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  hind  foot  by  any  of  various 
stimuli  that  have  the  character  of  threatening  the  part  with 
damage  causes  the  leg  to  be  flexed,  drawing  the  foot  up  by 
'  steady  maintained  contraction  of  the  flexors  of  the  ankle,  knee, 
and  hip.  In  this  reaction  the  reflex-arc  is  (under  schematic 
provisions  similar  to  those  mentioned  in  regard  to  the  scratch- 
reflex  schema)  (i)  the  receptive  neurone  (Fig.  39  B,  L),  noci-cep- 
tive,  from  the  foot  to  the  spinal  segment,  (ii)  the  motor  neurone 
(Fig.  39  B,  FC)  to  the  flexor  muscle,  e.g.  of  hip  (a  short  intra- 
spinal neurone;  a  Schalt-zelle  (v.  Monakow)  is  probably  ex- 
istent between  (i)  and  (ii)  but  omitted  for  simplicity).  Here, 
therefore,  there  is  an  arc  which  embouches  into  the  same  final 
common  path  FC  as  do  Ra  and  RyS,  Fig.  39  B.  The  motor 
neurone  FC  is  a  path  common  to  it  and  to  the  scratch-reflex 

Figure  42  (opposite).  —  Interference  of  the  reflex  from  the  skin  of  the  opposite  foot  with  the 
scratch-reflex.  FC,  the  flexor  muscle  of  the  left  hip  (Fig.  39  B,  fc).  r,  the  signal  line  the 
notch  in  which  marks  the  beginning,  continuance,  and  conclusion  of  a  skin  stimulation  of 
the  right  foot  (Fig.  39  B,  r.)  s,  signal  line  similarly  marking  the  period  of  stimulation  of 
the  skin  of  the  left  shoulder  (Fig.  39  B,  Ra).  The  ability  of  stimulus  s  to  produce  the 
scratch-reflex  takes  effect  only  on  concluding  stimulus  r  ;  that  is,  s  obtains  connection  with 
ihtjinal  common  path  (the  motor  neurone  of  the  flexor  muscle)  only  on  r's  relinquishing 
it.  Stimulus  r,  while  excluding  s  from  fc,  causes  slight  contraction  of  fc's  antagonist, 
and  coincident  slight  relaxation  of  fc  itself.  Time  in  fifths  of  seconds.  Read  from  left 
to  right. 


IV] 


ANTAGONISTIC   REFLEXES 


137 


138         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

arc;  both  these  arcs  employ  the  same  effector  organ,  namely, 
the  knee-flexor,  and  employ  it  by  the  common  medium  of 
the  final  path  FC.  But  though  the  channels  for  both  reflexes 
embouch  upon  the  same  final  common  path,  the  excitatory 
flexor  eff"ect  specific  to  each  differs  strikingly  in  the  two  cases. 
In  the  scratch-reflex  the  flexor  effect  is  an  intermittent  effect; 
in  the  noci-ceptive  flexion-reflex  the  flexor  effect  is  steady  and 
maintained.  The  accompanying  tracing  (Fig.  43)  shows  the 
result  of  conflict  between  the  two  reflexes.  The  one  reflex 
displaces  the  other  at  the  common  path.  Compromise  is  not 
evident.  The  scratch-reflex  is  set  aside  by  that  of  the  noci-cep- 
tive arc  from  the  homonymous  foot.  The  stimulation  which 
previously  sufficed  to  provoke  the  scratch-reflex  is  no  longer 
effective,  though  it  is  continued  all  the  time.  But  when  the 
stimulation  of  the  foot  is  discontinued  the  scratch-reflex  returns. 
In  that  respect,  although  there  is  no  enforced  inactivity  there  is 
an  interference  which  is  tantamount  to,  if  not  the  same  thing  as, 
inhibition.  Though  there  is  no  cessation  of  activity  in  the 
motor  neurone,  one  form  of  activity  that  was  being  impressed 
upon  it  is  cut  short  and  another  takes  its  place.  A  stimulation 
of  the  foot  too  weak  to  cause  more  than  a  minimal  reflex  will 
often  suffice  to  completely  interrupt,  or  cut  short,  or  prevent 
onset  of,  the  scratch-reflex. 

The  kernel  of  the  interference  between  the  homonymous 
flexion-reflex  and  the  scratch-reflex  is  that  both  employ  the 
same  final  common  path  FC  to  different  effect^ just  as  in 
the  interference  between  the  crossed  extension-reflex  and  the 
scratch-reflex.  Evidently,  the  homonymous  flexion-reflex  and 
the  crossed  extension-reflex  both  use  the  same  final  common 
path  FC.     And  they  use  it  to  different  effect.    The  motor  neurone 

Figure  43  (opposite).  —  Interference  between  the  reflex  action  of  the  left  hip  flexor,  FC,  caused 
by  the  nervous  arc  from  the  left  foot  (l,  Fig.  39  B)  and  the  scratch-reflex.  The  stimula- 
tion of  the  dorsal  skin  (Fig.  39  A)  inducing  the  scratch-reflex  began  at  the  beginning  of  the 
notch  in  the  signal  line  s,  and  continued  throughout  the  period  of  that  notch.  Later,  for  the 
period  marked  by  the  notch  in  signal  line  L,  the  stimulation  of  the  foot  was  made.  This 
latter  stimulation  interrupts  the  clonic  scratch-reflex  in  the  manner  shown.  The  time  is 
registered  above  in  fifths  of  seconds.  The  tracing  reads  from  left  to  right.  It  is  note- 
worthy that  the  interruption  of  the  scratch-reflex  by  the  foot-reflex  is  not  established 
directly  the  foot-stimulus  begins,  and  that  it  outlasts  for  a  short  time  the  application  of 
the  foot-stimulus. 


IV] 


ANTAGONISTIC    REFLEXES 


1-39 


140         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

to  the  flexor  of  the  knee  being  taken  as  representative  of  the 
final  common  path,  the  homonymous  flexion-reflex  excites  it 
to  discharging  activity,  but  the  crossed  extension-reflex  inhibits 
it  from  discharging.  Hence  if,  while  the  direct  flexion-reflex  is 
in  progress  the  crossed  foot  is  stimulated,  the  reflex  of  the  knee- 
flexor  is  inhibited.  The  crossed  extension-reflex  therefore  in- 
hibits not  only  the  scratch-reflex  but  also  the  homonymous 
flexion-reflex. 

Further,  in  all  these  interferences  between  reflexes  the  direc- 
tion taken  by  the  inhibition  is  reversible.  Thus,  the  scratch- 
reflex  is  not  only  liable  to  be  inhibited  by,  but  is  itself  able 
to  inhibit,  either  the  homonymous  flexion-reflex  or  the  crossed 
extension-reflex;  the  homonymous  flexion-reflex  is  not  only 
capable  of  being  inhibited  by  the  crossed  extension-reflex  (Fig. 
32,  p.  98),  but  conversely  in  its  turn  can  inhibit  the  crossed 
extension-reflex  (Figs.  33,  35,  p.  100).  These  interferences  are 
therefore  reversible  in  direction.  Certain  conditions  determine 
which  reflex  among  two  or  more  competing  ones  shall  obtain 
mastery  over  the  final  common  path  and  thus  obtain  expression. 

Therefore,  in  regard  to  the  final  common  path  FC  the 
reflexes  that  express  themselves  in  it  can  be  grouped  into  sets, 
namely  those  which  excite  it  in  one  way,  those  which  excite 
it  in  another  way,  and  those  which  inhibit  it.  The  reflexes 
composing  each  of  these  sets  stand  in  such  relation  to  reflexes 
of  the  same  set  that  they  are  with  them  "  allied  reflexes."  But 
a  reflex  belonging  to  any  one  of  these  sets  stands  in  such 
relation  to  a  reflex  belonging  to  one  of  the  other  sets  that  it 
is  in  regard  to  the  latter  an  "  antagonistic  "  reflex.  This  cor- 
relation of  reflexes  about  the  flexor  neurone  in  the  leg  so  that 
some  reflexes  are  mutually  allied  and  some  are  mutually  antag- 
onistic in  regard  to  that  neurone,  may  serve  as  a  paradigm  of 
the  correlation  of  reflexes  about  every  final  common  path,  e.  g. 
about  every  motor  nerve  to  skeletal  muscle.^^ 

As  to  the  intimate  nature  of  the  mechanism  which  thus,  by 
summation  or  by  interference,  gives  co-ordination  where  neurones 
converge  upon  a  common  path  it  is  difficult  to  surmise.  In  the 
central  nervous  system  of  vertebrates,  afl"erent  neurones  A  and 


IV]  SUMMATION   AND    INTERFERENCE  141 

B  in  their  convergence  toward  and  impingement  upon  another 
neurone  Z,  towards  which  they  conduct,  do  not  make  Any 
lateral  connection  directly  one  with  the  other  —  at  least  there 
seems  no  clear  evidence  that  they  do.  It  seems  then  that  the 
only  structural  link  between  A  and  B  is  neurone  Z  itself.  Z 
itself  should  therefore  be  the  field  of  coalition  of  A  and  B  if 
they  transmit  **  allied  "  reflexes. 

It  was  argued  above  (Lecture  III),  from  the  morphology  of 
the  perikaryon,  that  it  must  form,  in  numerous  cases,  a  nodal 
point  in  the  conductive  lines  provided  by  the  neurone.  The 
work  of  Ramon-y-Cajal,  van  Gehuchten,  v.  Lenhoss^k,  and 
others  with  the  methods  of  Golgi  and  Ehrlich,  establishes  as 
a  concept  of  the  neurone  in  general  that  it  is  a  conductive  unit 
wherein  a  number  of  branches  (dendrites)  converge  toward,  meet 
at,  and  coalesce  in  a  single  outgoing  stem  (axone).  Through  this 
tree-shaped  structure  the  nervous  impulses  flow,  like  the  water 
in  a  tree,  from  roots  to  stem.  The  conduction  does  not  normally 
run  in  the  reverse  direction.  The  place  of  junction  of  the  den- 
drites with  one  another  and  with  the  axone  is  commonly  the 
perikaryon.  This  last  is  therefore  a  nodal  point  in  the  conduc- 
tive system.  But  it  is  a  nodal  point  of  particular  quality.  It  is 
not  a  nodal  point  where  lines  meet  to  cross  one  another,  nor 
one  where  one  line  splits  into  many.  It  is  a  nodal  point  where 
conductive  lines  run  together  into  one  which  is  the  continuation 
of  them  all.  It  is  a  reduction  point  in  the  system  of  lines.  The 
perikaryon  with  its  convergent  dendrites  is  therefore  just  such 
a  structure  as  spatial  summation  and  immediate  induction  would 
demand.  The  neurone  Z  may  well,  therefore,  be  the  field  of 
coalition,  and  the  organ  where  the  summational  and  inductive 
processes  occur.  And  the  morphology  of  the  neurone  as  a 
whole  is  seen  to  be  just  such  as  we  should  expect,  arguing  from 
the  principle  of  the  common  path. 

With  the  phenomenon  of  "  interference  "  the  question  is  more 
difficult.  There  it  is  not  clear  that  the  field  of  antagonism  is 
within  the  neurone  Z  itself.  The  field  may  be  synaptic.  We 
have  the  demonstration  by  Verworn^o^  that  the  interference  pro- 
duced by  A  at  Z  for  impulses  from  B  is  not  accompanied  by 


142         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES      [Lect. 

any  obvious  change  in  excitability  of  the  axone  of  Z.  Z,  if  itself 
the  seat  of  inhibition,  might  have  been  expected  to  exhibit  that 
inhibition  throughout  its  extent.  This,  as  tested  by  its  axone, 
it  does  not  do.  There  exist,  it  is  true,  older  experiments  by 
Uspensky,*^  Belmondo  and  Oddi,^^^  ^^^^^  according  to  which 
the  threshold  of  direct  excitability  of  the  motor  root  is  lowered 
by  stimulation  of  the  afferent  root.  This  points  to  an  exten- 
sion of  the  facilitation  effect  through  the  whole  motor  neurone, 
conversely  to  Verworn's  demonstration  for  central  inhibition. 
Verworn's  experiment  and  its  result  is  very  clear.  It  leads  us 
to  search  for  some  other  mechanism  common  to  A  and  B  to 
which  might  be  attributable  their  mutual  influence  on  each 
other's  reactions.  But  if  we  admit  the  conception,  argued 
above  (Lecture  I),  that  at  the  nexus  between  A  and  Z,  z.  e.  at 
synapse  A  Z,  and  similarly  between  B  and  Z,  i.  e,  at  synapse 
B  Z,  there  exists  a  surface  of  separation,  a  membrane  in  the 
physical  sense,  a  further  consequence  seems  inferable.  Suppose 
a  number  of  different  neurones  A,  B,  C,  etc.,  each  conducting 
through  its  own  synapse  upon  a  neurone  Z.  The  synapses 
A  Z,  B  Z,  C  Z,  etc.,  are  all  surfaces  or  membranes  into  which  Z 
enters  as  a  factor  common  to  them  all.  A  change  of  state 
induced  in  neurone  Z  might  be  expected  to  affect  the  surface 
condition  or  membrane  at  all  of  the  synapses,  since  the  condition 
of  Z  is  a  factor  common  to  all  those  membranes.  Therefore 
a  change  of  state  (excitatory  or  inhibitory)  induced  in  Z  by 
any  of  the  neurones  A,  B,  C,  etc.,  playing  upon  it  would  enter 
as  a  condition  into  the  nervous  transmission  at  the  other  synap- 
ses from  the  other  collateral  neurones.  In  harmony  with  this  is 
the  spread  of  refractory  state  in  the  neurones  as  mentioned  above 
(p.  122).  A  change  in  neurone  Z  induced  by  neurone  A,  playing 
upon  it,  in  that  case  seems  to  affect  its  point  of  nexus  with  the 
other  neurones  B,  C,  etc.,  also.  It  is  conceivable  that  the  phe- 
nomena of  interference  may  be  based  in  part  at  least  on  such 
a  condition.  The  neurone  threshold  of  Z  for  stimulation  through 
B  will  be  to  some  extent  a  function  of  events  at  synapses  A  Z. 

Partial  interference.     It  has  to  be  remembered,  however,  that 
the  total  final  common  path,  although  a  functional  unity,  is  often, 


IV]  THE   COMMON   PATH  143 

especially  in  compound  reflexes,  a  complex  one.  It  frequently 
happens  that  the  set  of  final  paths  of  one  complex  reflex  is 
partly  coextensive  with  the  set  of  final  common  paths  of  another 
reflex.  With  two  complex  reflexes  it  can  happen  that  the 
reflexes  are  "allied  reflexes"  in  regard  to  one  part  of  their 
multiple  final  common  path  and  are  antagonistic  reflexes  in 
regard  to  another  part  of  it.  We  may  illustrate  this  from  the 
scratch-reflex  again.  The  scratch-reflex  was  mentioned  above 
as  being  unilateral.  That  is  not  strictly  the  case.  It  is  true 
that  if  the  right  scapular  region  be  stimulated,  the  right  hind 
leg  scratches ;  and  if  the  left  scapular  region  be  stimulated  the 
left  hind  leg  scratches.  But  if  both  shoulders  be  stimulated  at 
the  same  time,  one  or  the  other  leg  scratches,  but  not  the  two 
together.  This  shows  that  the  scratch-reflex,  though  at  first 
sight  it  appears  unilateral,  is  not  strictly  so.  Suppose  the  left 
shoulder  stimulated,  the  left  leg  then  scratches ;  but  if  the  right 
leg  is  examined  it  is  found  to  present  slight  steady  extension 
with  some  abduction. 

This  extension  of  the  crossed  hind  leg  which  accompanies 
the  scratching  movement  of  the  homonymous  hind  leg  contrib- 
utes to  support  the  animal  on  three  legs  while  it  scratches  with 
the  fourth.  Suppose  stimulation  at  the  left  shoulder  evoking 
the  scratching  movement  of  the  left  leg,  and  the  skin  of  the 
right  shoulder  then  appropriately  and  strongly  stimulated. 
This  latter  stimulus  often  inhibits  the  scratching  movement  in 
the  opposite  leg  and  starts  it  in  its  own.^^  That  is,  the  stimulus 
at  the  right  shoulder  not  only  sets  the  flexor  muscles  of  the  leg 
of  its  own  side  into  scratching  action,  but  it  inhibits  the  flexor 
muscles  of  the  opposite  leg,  because  with  excitation  of  the  ex- 
tensors of  the  latter  leg  goes  inhibition  of  their  antagonists, 
the  flexors.  The  motor  neurones  of  the  flexor  muscles  of  the 
left  leg  are  part  of  the  final  common  path  not  only  of  the 
scratch-reflex  of  the  left  shoulder,  but  also  of  the  scratch-reflex 
of  the  right  shoulder ;  but  in  the  former  case  the  final  common 
path  is  thrown  into  rhythmic  discharging  activity,  in  the  latter 
case  it  is  steadily  inhibited  from  discharging  activity. 

Again,  the  homonymous  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  leg  (spinal 


144        INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

dog)  is  only  the  main  part  of  a  larger  complex  reflex  which  is 
bilateral  (Fig.  37),  and  consists  of  flexion  of  the  same  side  leg 
and  extension  of  the  crossed  leg  (the  crossed  extension-reflex). 
This  being  so,  the  mutual  relation  between  the  complete  scratch- 
reflex,  e.  g.  of  left  foot,  and  the  complete  noci-ceptive  reflex  of 
the  same  foot,  is  that  the  homonymous  uncrossed  parts  of  each 
reflex  interfere  and  are  related  mutually  as  antagonistic  reflexes ; 
but  the  crossed  parts  of  each  reflex  coalesce  in  excitation  of 
the  extensor  neurones  and  inhibition  of  the  flexor  neurones  of 
the  right  leg,  and  are  related  mutually  as  allied  reflexes. 

It  is  the  transference  of  the  final  common  path  from  the 
group  of  one  set  of  reflexes  to  another  which  constitutes  the 
change  which  occurs  at  each  step  of  the  orderly  sequence  of 
reaction  that  we  see  normally  succeed  each  other  in  animal 
behaviour  —  leaving  aside  all  question  of  consciousness  in  rela- 
tion to  the  sequence.  This  transference  is  most  obvious  when 
the  sets  of  reflexes  between  which  the  final  common  path  is 
exchanged  are  antagonistic  reflexes.  Two  classes  of  this  kind  of 
case  of  specially  common  occurrence  are  "  alternating  reflexes  " 
and  "  compensatory  reflexes  "  (Lecture  VI.). 

Number  of  common  paths.  The  interaction  of  reflexes  has 
been  here  so  far  spoken  of  chiefly  in  regard  to  the  final 
common  path,  as  if  the  arcs  of  reflexes  met  at  the  final 
common  path  only.  But,  as  stated  above,  reflex-arcs,  espe- 
cially the  longer  ones  and  those  commencing  in  receptors 
far  apart,  converge  and  meet  to  some  extent  before  they 
reach  their  final  common  path.  The  receptive  neurones,  i.  e. 
private  paths  of  the  receptors,  usually  —  perhaps  always  — 
reach  internuncial  paths  (J.  Hunter,  17 yd),  which  in  turn 
conduct  and  converge  to  final  paths  or  to  further  internuncial 
paths.  The  internuncial  paths  are  thus  themselves  in  various 
degrees  common  to  groups  of  receptive  neurones  impinging 
upon  them.  They  are  therefore  themselves,  to  some  extent, 
common  paths^  There  can  be  little  doubt  that  in  the  scratch- 
reflex  the  long  descending  proprio-spinal  neurone  (Fig.  39  B, 
Pa  or  P/3)  is  connected  not  with  one  but  with  a  whole  group  of 
afferent  neurones  (private  paths)  from  the  scalptor  receptors  in 


that 


WEAK    REFLEXES   MAY   BE   NEUTRAL         145 


hat  part  of  the  skin-field  of  the  scratch-reflex  which  corresponds 

i)vith  its  own  spinal  segment.  Its  internuncial  path  is  therefore 
;ommon  to  impulses  transmitted  to  the  central  organ  by  many 
receptive  paths.  Again,  the  structure  of  the  retina  (Cajal),  olfac- 
tory bulb  (Cajal),  etc.,  gives  evidence  that  the  conducting  fibres 
of  whole  groups  of  receptors  impinge  together  upon  individual 
neurones  of  the  next  relay.  Thalamic  neurones  form  a  path 
upon  which  the  dorsal-column-fillet  and  spino-cerebellar-pedun- 
cular    paths   converge.     Each    internuncial    path   is   therefore 

Iisually,  to  some  extent,  a  common  path,^^  just  as  usually  the 
eceptive  neurone,  /.  e.  private  path,  itself  is  common  to  a  small 
lumber  of  receptors.  The  ultimate  path,  therefore,  differs  from 
he  intermediate  paths  only  in  that  it  exhibits  communism  in 
he  highest  degree;  it  is  to  distinguish  it  from  internuncial 
common  paths  that  it  was  termed  above  the  final  common 
path. 

Since  each  instance  of  convergence  of  two  or  more  afferent 
neurones  upon  a  third,  which  in  regard  to  them  is  efferent, 
affords,  as  shown  above,  an  opportunity  for  coalition  or  inter- 
ference of  their  actions,  each  structure  at  which  it  occurs  is  a 
mechanism  for  co-ordination?^ 

Whatever  may  be  the  intimate  nature  of  this  mechanism 
which  gives  co-ordination  by  the  formation  of  a  common 
path  from  tributary  paths,  such  common  paths  exist  in  ex- 
traordinary profusion  in  the  architecture  of  the  gray-centred 
nervous  system  of  vertebrates.  Two  features  of  that  system 
indicate  this  clearly.  Enumerations  by  Donaldson  and  his  co- 
workers 264, 265  show  that  the  afferent  fibres  (private  paths)  enter- 
ing the  human  spinal  cord  three  times  outnumber  the  efferent 
(final  common  paths)  which  leave  it.  Add  the  cranial  nerves 
and  the  so-called  optic  nerves  (in  the  latter,  of  course,  formation 
of  common  paths  having  already  begun  in  the  retina  the  afferent 
paths  are  reduced  in  proportion)  and  the  afferent  fibres  may  be 
taken  to  be  five  times  more  numerous  than  the  efferent.  The  re- 
ceptor system  bears,  therefore,  to  the  efferent  paths  the  relation 
of  the  wide  ingress  of  a  funnel  to  the  narrow  egress.  Further, 
each  receptor  stands  in  connection  not  with  one  efferent  only  but 


146         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

with  many  —  perhaps  with  all,  though  as  to  some  of  these  only- 
through  synapses  of  high  resistance.  The  simile  to  a  funnel 
will  therefore  be  bettered  by  supposing  that  within  the  general 
systematic  funnel,  of  which  the  base  is  five  times  wider  than  the 
egress,  the  conducting  paths  from  each  receptor  may  be  repre- 
sented as  a  funnel  inverted  so  that  its  wider  end  is  more  or  less 
coextensive  with  the  whole  plane  of  emergence  of  the  final 
common  paths.^^  This  gives  some  idea  of  the  enormous  forma- 
tion of  common  paths  from  tributary  paths  which  must  take 
place. 

Again,  there  is  the  accredited  fact  that  under  poisoning  by 
strychnine  a  muscle  can  be  excited  from  practically  any  afferent 
nerve  in  the  body ;  in  other  words,  that  each  final  common  path 
is  in  connection  with  practically  each  one  of  all  the  receptors 
of  the  body.  It  is  not  necessary  to  accept  this  literally ;  even 
if  approximately  true,  it  shows  the  profusion  in  which  common 
paths  exist. 

Mutual  indifference  between  reflexes.  In  view  of  such  con- 
siderations the  question  arises,  Are  there  in  the  body  no  re- 
flexes absolutely  neutral  and  indifferent  one  to  another?  That 
is,  in  regard  to  any  one  reflex  using  a  given  common  path 
cannot  another  reflex  be  found  which  is  wholly  separate  from 
it,  and  neither  allied  with  it  nor  antagonistic  to  it?  It  was 
pointed  out  above  that  the  coalition  between  scratch-reflexes 
gradually  decreases  as  the  interval  between  the  receptive  points 
at  the  skin  surface  becomes  wider.  Whether  coalition  fades 
into  mere  indifference  or  passes  over  into  antagonism  my  own 
observations  do  not  answer.  But  there  are  reflexes  that  do 
in  the  spinal  dog  appear  neutral  and  indifferent  to  the  scratch- 
reflex.  For  instance,  a  weak  reflex  of  the  tail  may  be  ob- 
tained without  any  obvious  interference  between  it  and  the 
scratch-reflex.  The  stronger  two  reflexes  are,  the  less  do  they 
remain  neutral  one  to  another.  Thus,  a  weak  reflex  may  be 
excited  from  the  tail  of  the  spinal  dog  without  interference  with 
the  stepping-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  ;  but  a  strong  reflex  (strong 
stimulus)  in  the  tail  inhibits  (Goltz)  the  stepping-reflex.  The 
spatial  field  of  response  of  a  reflex  increases  with  its  intensity. 


1^  THE  FINAL  COMMON  PATH  147 

wo  reflexes  may  be  neutral  to  each  other  when  both  are  weak, 
ut  may  interfere  when  either  or  both  are  strong ;  when  weak 
they  remain  "  local." 

But  to  show  that  reflexes  may  be  neutral  to  each  other  in 
a  spinal  dog  is  not  evidence  that  they  will  be  neutral  in  the 
animal  with  its  whole  nervous  system  intact  and  unmutilated. 
It  is  a  cardinal  feature  of  the  construction  of  the  higher  verte- 
brate nervous  system  that  longer  indirect  reflex-arcs,  attached 
as  extra  circuits  to  the  shorter  direct  ones,  all  pass  through  the 
brain.  With  those  former  intact  the  number  of  reflexes  neutral 
one  to  another  might  be  fewer.  In  presence  of  the  arcs  of  the 
great  projicient  receptors  (Lect.  IX)  and  the  brain  there  can  be  few 
receptive  points  in  the  body  whose  activities  are  totally  indif- 
ferent one  to  another.  Correlation  of  the  reflexes  from  points 
widely  apart  is  the  crowning  contribution  of  the  brain  towards 
the  nervous  integration  of  the  individual. 

Our  conception  comes  therefore  to  this.  About  any  final 
common  path  a  great  number  of,  or  all,  the  receptive  arcs  of  the 
nervous  system  are  arranged  and  are  divisible  into  sets  that  do  not 
act  alike  upon  it.  It  might  at  first  be  thought  that  there  would 
be  simply  two  such  sets,  namely,  those  that  excite  it  and  those 
that  inhibit  it.  But  it  must  be  remembered  that  we  are  only 
at  the  beginning  of  knowledge  of  differences  of  time-relations 
between  different  type-reflexes.  Thus  (Lectures  II,  III)  at  the 
I  knee  of  the  spinal  dog  the  time-relations  of  the  extensor-thrust 
are  vastly  different  from  those  of  the  crossed  extension-reflex, 
and  these  again  from  the  extensor  tonus  that  supports  the  knee- 
jerk,  and  these  again  from  the  scratch-reflex,  and  so  on.  Of 
the  reflexes  that  excite  a  final  common  path  some  evidently 
excite  it  in  a  manner  very  diff'erent  from  that  in  which  some 
others  excite  it;  their  excitations  if  concurrent  interfere.  We 
must  therefore  allow  that  the  sets  may  be  more  than  two,  if  the 
criterion  for  distinguishing  the  sets  be  interference,  i.  e.  inter- 
ruption, displacement,  or  extinction  at  the  final  common  path 
of  one  reflex  by  another. 

The  final  common  path  is  therefore  an  instrument  passive  in 


148         INTERACTION   BETWEEN   REFLEXES     [Lect. 

the  hands  of  certain  groups  of  reflex-paths.  I  have  attempted 
to  depict  this  very  simply  in  Fig.  44.  There  certain  type- 
reflexes  are  indicated  by  lines  representing  their  paths.  The 
final  common  path  (fc)  selected  is  the  motor  neurone  of  the 
vasto-crureus  of  the  dog  or  cat.  Reflexes  that  act  as  "  allied 
reflexes  "  on  FC  are  represented  as  having  their  terminals  joined 


I 


Figure  44.  —  Explanation  mainly  in  text,    s  stands  for  scratch-receptor,  e  and/ are  extensor 
and  flexor  muscles  of  knee  respectively. 


I 


IV] 


THE  FINAL  COMMON   PATH 


149 


together  next  the  final  common  path.  Reflexes  with  excitatory- 
effect  (+  sign)  are  brought  together  on  the  left,  those  with  in- 
hibitory (—  sign)  on  the  right.  Of  the  reflex  pairs  formed  by 
the  two  reflexes  which  two  symmetrical  receptive  points,  one ' 
right  and  one  left,  yield  in  regard  to  the  final  common  path,  one 
of  the  pair  only  is  represented,  in  order  to  simplify  the  diagram. 
To  have  a  further  indication  of  the  reflexes  playing  upon  FC, 
all  that  is  required  is  to  add  to  the  reflexes  indicated  in  the 
diagram  for  FC,  a  set  of  reflexes  similar  to  those  given  in  the  dia- 
gram for  Fc',  for  they  must  be  added  if  the  remaining  members 
of  the  right  and  left  reflex  pairs  from  various  parts  of  the  body 
be  taken  into  account.  It  is  noteworthy  that  in  many  instances 
the  end-effect  of  a  spinal  reflex  initiated  from  a  surface  point  on 
one  side  is  bilateral  and  takes  effect  at  symmetrical  parts,  but  is 
opposite  in  kind  at  those  two  parts,  e.  g.  is  inhibition  at  one 
of  them,  excitation  at  the  other.  Hence  reflexes  initiated  from 
points  corresponding  one  with  the  other  in  the  two  halves  of 
the  body  are  commonly  antagonistic. 


ISO.  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 


LECTURE   V 

COMPOUND  REFLEXES:    SIMULTANEOUS   COMBINATION 

Argument :  Combination  of  reflexes  simultaneously  proceeding.  Spread 
of  reflex-response  about  a  focus.  Gray  matter  and  lines  of  reflex 
resistance.  "Short"  reflexes  and  "long"  reflexes.  Rules  decipher- 
able in  the  spread  of  reflex  reaction.  Pfltiger's  "  laws  "  of  spinal 
irradiation.  The  "  reflex  figure."  Variability  of  reflex  result.  Irra- 
diation of  a  reflex  attaches  itself  to  the  problem  of  the  simultaneous 
combination  of  reflexes.  Co-ordination  of  reflex  result  obtains  even 
when  large  mixed  afferent  nerve-trunks  are  stimulated.  The  move- 
ment excited  by  stimulation  of  the  motor  spinal  nerve-root  does  not 
really  resemble  a  movement  evoked  reflexly  or  by  the  will.  Extent 
of  simultaneous  combinations  of  reflexes.  Simultaneous  stimuli 
arrange  themselves  naturally  in  constellations  in  which  some  com- 
ponent is  usually  of  pre-eminent  intensity.  The  resulting  compound 
reaction  has  both  positive  and  negative  sides. 

A  LARGE  part  of  co-ordination  consists  in  the  orderly  com- 
bining of  reflexes.  In  studying  this  co-ordination  vi^e  have  to 
deal  with  and  discriminate  between  simultaneous  combinations 
and  successive  combinations  of  reflexes.  We  may  proceed  to 
attempt  the  former  problem. 

Irradiation.  If  by  appropriate  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the 
foot,  say  by  unipolar  faradization  of  a  spot  of  the  plantar  skin 
of  a  digit,  the  ordinary  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  of  the 
dog  be  evoked,  the  extent  of  the  reflex  increases  with  increase 
in  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus.  The  reflex-effect  spreads  over 
a  larger  and  larger  field,  irradiating  as  it  were  in  various  direc- 
tions from  a  focus  of  reflex-discharge  which  takes  effect  on  the 
limb  itself. 

The  centrifugal  discharge  elicited  by  any  reflex  seems  as 
regards  its  spatial  distribution  to  be  focussed  about  a  centre 
round  which  its  irradiation  varies  according  to  circumstance. 
In  the  scratch-reflex  the  pretibial  muscles  that  dorso-flex  the 
ankle  seem  to   he  at  the  focus  of  the   motor  discharge.     In 


I 


]  COMBINATIONS   OF   REFLEXES  151 


01 


he  "  flexion-reflex,"  if  the  reaction  evoked  is  very  weak  a  band 
of  the  deep  inner  hamstring  muscle  has  often  in  my  experience 
emed  the  only  part  of  the  musculature  thrown  into  action, 
n  the  other  hand,  when  the  reflex  is  evoked  with  medium 
strength  it  can  often  be  seen  that  after  the  reflex  (the  exc-iting 
stimulation  being  continued  unaltered)  has  been  in  progress  for 
a  few  seconds,  flexion  at  hip  adds  itself  to  the  flexion  at  the 
^^nee  (see  Fig.  45).     And  by  strong  stimulation,  strong  flexion 
I^Bit  hip  occurs  together  with  that  at  knee  and  practically  from 
^^Hhe  very  outset.     In  my  experience  the  condition  of  **  spinal 
^^Bhock"  is  very  favourable  for  noting. the  seat  of  the  focus  of 
|^|the  motor  discharge  in  a  reflex,  because  in  that  condition  it 
happens  often  that  the  piece  of  musculature  which  is  at  focus 
of  the  discharge  is  the  only  one  which  can  be  got  to  give  the 
reflex-response.     It  seems  possible  in  this  way  to  determine  what 
reflex  in,  for  instance,  a  "  spinal "  monkey  corresponds  with  this 
or  that  reflex  in  a  "  spinal  "  dog.     In  the  monkey  the  severity 
and  long  duration  of  spinal  shock  allows  merely  the  focal  reply 
in  the  musculature.     Thus  a  feeble  tightening  of  a  part  of  a 
amstring  muscle  in  the  *'  spinal"  monkey  affords  fair  evidence, 
response  to  a  stimulus  of  the  foot,  that  the  flexor-reflex  —  for 
e  full  extent  of  which  one  must  turn  to  the  "  spinal  dog  "  —  is 
oked.     In  man  spinal  shock  seems  still  more  severe  and  last- 
ng  than  in  the  monkey.     The  situation  of  the  weak  brief  con- 
actions  evoked  can  still  reveal  which  they  correspond  with 
among  reflexes  better  open  to  study  in  the  lower  mammals. 

The  more  intense  the  spinal  reflex  —  apart  from  strychnine 
and  similar  convulsant  poisoning — the  wider,  as  a  general  rule, 
the  extent  to  which  the  motor  discharge  spreads  around  its  focal 
area.  Thus,  as  stimulation  of  the  planta  causing  the  flexion- 
reflex  is  increased  there  is  added  ^^2»  ^'^  to  the  flexion  of  the 
homonymous  hind  limb  extension  of  the  crossed  hind  limb,  then 
in  the  homonymous  fore  limb  extension  at  elbow  and  retraction 
at  shoulder,  then  at  the  crossed  fore  limb  flexion  at  elbow, 
extension  at  wrist,  and  some  protraction  at  shoulder;  also  turning 
of  the  head  toward  the  homonymous  side,  and  often  opening  of 
the  mouth,  also  lateral  deviation  of  the  tail. 


152  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

According  to  circumstance,  especially  according  to  intensity 
of  stimulation,  the  field  of  end-effect  of  the  flexion-reflex  may 
vary  from  a  minute  field  occupying  part  of  a  flexor  muscle  of 
the  knee  to  a  field  including  musculature  in  all  four  limbs  and 
neck  and  head  and  tail. 

That  the  reaction  should  spread  in  its  spatial  extent  is  not 
surprising.  The  afferent  neurone  on  entering  the  central  organ, 
the  spinal  cord,  enters  a  vast  network  of  conduction  of  paths 
interlacing  in  all  directions.  A  glance  at  any  Weigert  prepara- 
tion of  the  spinal  cord  shows  a  tangle  of  branching  nerve-fibres, 
the  richness  and  intricacy  of  which  seems  practically  infinite. 
Into  this  forest  the  receptive  neurone  conducts  the  impulses, 
and  can  itself  be  traced,  breaking  up  into  many  divisions  that 
pass  in  many  directions  and  to  various  distances.  And  this  web 
of  conductive  channels  into  which  the  centripetal  impulses  of  the 
reflex  are  thus  launched  is  known  to  be  practically  a  continuum 
in  the  sense  that  no  part  of  the  nervous  system  is  isolated  from 
the  rest.  "  A  group  of  nerve-cells  disconnected  from  the  other 
nerve-tissues  of  the  body,  as  the  muscles  or  glands  are  discon- 
nected from  each  other,  would  be  without  physiological  sig- 
nificance. To  understand  the  physiology  of  the  nervous  system 
it  is  important  to  keep  in  mind  the  fact  that  by  histology  it  is 
found  to  be  continuous  throughout  its  entire  extent."  ^^  And 
there  is  the  generally  accredited  statement  that  on  exhibition  of 
strychnine  centripetal  impulses  poured  in  via  any  afferent  nerve, 
excite  reflex-discharge  over  the  efferent  channels  of  the  whole 
nerve-system.  This,  even  if  not  strictly  true,  is  sufficiently  ap- 
proximate to  the  truth  to  show  the  enormous  interconnections 
between  any  afferent  channel  and  the  congeries  of  arcs  of  the 

Figure  45. —  Maintenance  of  the  scratch-reflex  A  and  the  flexion-reflex  B  respectively  under 
unipolar  faradic  stimulation  of  comparable  intensity.  The  diffuse  electrode  (anode)  was 
on  the  fore  limb  in  each  case;  the  primary  circuit  and  its  rate  of  interruption  was  the  same, 
and  the  secondary  coil  of  the  inductorium  remained  at  the  same  distance  from  the  pri- 
mary. For  the  scratch-reflex  the  needle  electrode  was  set  in  the  skin  of  the  loin,  for  the 
flexion-reflex  in  the  plantar  skin  of  the  outermost  digit.  After  giving  28  beats  the 
scratch-reflex  died  out,  having  lasted  about  seven  seconds.  Further  continuation  of 
the  stimulation  was  cut  short  as  useless  after  four  more  seconds.  The  flexion-reflex,  on 
the  other  hand,  is  in  full  intensity  at  the  end  of  the  13th  second  of  continued  application 
of  the  stimulus,  and  its  amplitude  is  perfectly  maintained  at  the  end  of  the  20th,  although 
the  reflex  is  rather  tremulant.     At  the  44th  second,  when  it  has  become  more  tremulant 


V] 


IRRADIATION 


153 


154  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

whole  central  nervous  system.  It  is  therefore  not  surprising 
that  the  reflex  reaction  should  spread.  On  the  other  hand,  the 
data  leave  unexplained  certain  features  of  the  spread.  How  is 
it  that  the  spread,  as  the  reflex  is  intensified,  does  not  extend 
everywhere,  as  it  is  said  to  do  in  strychnine  poisoning?  How 
is  it  that  in  the  flexion-reflex,  of  the  cat  for  instance,  the  spread 
does  not  extend  to  the  muscles  of  the  pinna  of  the  ear?  It  is 
easy  from  certain  parts  to  obtain  the  brisk  reflex  retraction  of 
the  pinna.  Yet  in  my  experience  the  stimulation  of  the  foot 
that  causes  the  flexion-reflex  and  all  its  various  irradiations  may 
be  pushed  without  evoking  retraction  or  other  movement  of 
the  pinna.  In  other  words,  the  irradiations  of  the  reflex  occur 
along  certain  lines  only  and  not  along  others,  and  the  line  to 
the  pinna  is  of  these  latter. 

Evidently  the  irradiation  from  each  entrant  path  tends  to  run 
in  certain  directions  and  not  in  all.  This  fact  is  sometimes  stated 
in  the  form  that  gray  matter  offers  to  the  entrant  path  lines  of 
conduction  possessing  different  degrees  of  resistance.  To  say 
this  merely  of  course  restates  the  fact  in  terms  suggesting 
analogy  between  nerve-paths  and  electric  circuits.  Before  the 
Golgi  and  methylene-blue  methods  had  thrown  doubt  on  the 
intricate  forest  of  nerve-fibres  in  the  gray  matter  being  a  net- 
work structurally  continuous  in  all  directions,  as  supposed  from 
the  Gerlach  preparations  and  the  universal  irradiation  under 
strychnine,  the  diff'erences  in  conductive  resistance  were  attrib- 
uted mainly  to  diff'erences  in  the  length  of  the  network  to 
be  traversed  by  some  reflexes  as  compared  with  others.  The 
longer  that  path  in  the  gray  matter  the  higher  was  thought  to 
be  the  resistance.  Evidence  indicating  slow  travel  of  impulses 
in  gray  matter  was  taken  as  evidence  of  resistance  in  gray 
matter.  In  certain  reactions  the  impulses  were  supposed  to 
have  very  long  paths  of  travel  in  gray  matter.  Thus,  impulses 
of  pain  were  supposed  to  ascend  along  the  spinal  gray  matter  to 
the  brain.  The  path  of  impulses  connected  with  pain  does  plunge 
into  the  gray  matter  very  soon  after  entering  the  spinal  cord ; 
it  then,  probably  after  a  short  course,  emerges  into  the  lateral 
white  columns,  preponderantly  of  the  side  crossed  from  that  on 


V]  NEURONE-THRESHOLD  155 

which  it  entered.  This  short  in-and-out  traverse  of  the  spinal 
gray  matter  seems  typical  of  all  paths  in  the.  gray  matter ;  they 
are  probably  all  quite  short.^^  If  all  synapses  lie  in  the  gray 
matter,  each  path  where  it  involves  a  passage  from  one  link  to 
another  of  the  neural  chain  must  enter  the  gray  matter  to  es- 
tablish its  linkage;    it  probably  soon  emerges  thence  again. 

Neurone -threshold.  But  one  finds  still  very  generally  ex- 
pressed the  view  that  the  differences  of  resistance  to  irradiation 
in  different  directions  are  referable  to  different  conductive  re- 
sistance offered  by  different  fibres  in  the  gray  matter.  The 
different  resistance  seems  more  probably  referable  to  differences 
in  the  facility  of  conduction  at  different  synapses.  At  each 
synapse  there  is  a  neurone-threshold.^®^  At  each  synapse  a 
small  quantity  of  energy,  freed  in  transmission,  acts  as  a  releas- 
ing force  to  a  fresh  store  of  energy  not  along  a  homogeneous 
train  of  conducting  material  as  in  a  nerve-fibre  pure  and  simple, 
but  across  a  barrier  which  whether  lower  or  higher  is  always  to 
some  extent  a  barrier.  There  is  abundant  evidence  that  different 
synapses  differ  from  one  another.  That  neurones  should  differ 
in  the  threshold  value  of  the  stimulus  necessary  to  excite  them 
seems  only  natural.  The  arguments  adduced  by  Goldscheider 
point  in  this  same  direction.  Many  of  the  phenomena  consid- 
ered in  the  first  three  lectures  are  easiest  explicable  by  such 
differences.  The  distinctions  between  different  synapses  in  re- 
gard to  ease  of  alteration  by  strychnine  and  by  tetanus  toxin 
emphasize  this  probability  further.  On  this  view  the  fact  that 
irradiation  of  a  reflex  reaction  spreads  along  certain  conductive 
arcs  more  readily  than  along  others,  can  be  schematically  figured 
as  in  the  diagram  (Fig.  46).  A  receptive  neurone  A  enters  the 
cord  and  forms  synaptic  connections  with  three  neurones,  the 
neurone-threshold  at  the  synapse  with  one  of  the  neurones  is 
higher  than  that  at  the  synapses  with  the  others.  The  threshold 
heights  (resistances)  are  represented  by  whole  numbers,  two  and 
one  respectively.  Each  of  the  intraspinal  neurones  in  its  turn 
forms  two  synaptic  connections  with  two  neurones,  and  in  these 
cases  also  the  thresholds  at  the  synapses  are  of  different  heights, 
numerically,  two  and  one  respectively.     On  the  view  that  the 


156 


COMPOUND   REFLEXES 


[Lect. 


Figure  46.  —  Explanation  in  text. 

action  of  one  neurone  upon  the  next  is  that  of  a  releasing  force 
liberating  a  potential  system  across  a  barrier  whose  resistance 
we  do  not  exactly  know,  it  is  impossible  to  predict  how  the  re- 
sistance will  sum  along  the  whole  conductive  chain.  It  is  clear 
that  although  the  total  resistance  of  the  reflex-arc  A  B  may  be 
numerically  represented  by  i,  the  resistance  along  A  D  need 
not,  on  the  numerical  values  assigned  to  the  synapses  in  the 
diagram  (Fig,  46),  sum  to  the  value  4.  Yet  it  is  also  clear  that 
the  threshold  for  any  whole  arc  cannot  be  lower  than  the 
highest  individual  threshold  in  it.  Further,  the  individual 
thresholds  will  tend  to  sum,  for  an  excitation  of  neurone  A 
just  sufficient  to  excite  neurone  a  is  hardly  likely  to  excite 
a  sufficiently  to  overcome  the  threshold  of  synapse  a  D.  Thus, 
with  even  small  grades  of  difference  of  threshold  at  different 
synapses,  large  differences  in  the  conductive  facility  of  different 
reflex-arcs  can  be  established. 

Similarly,  an  additive  influence  of  the  threshold  will  make  a 
reflex-chain  consisting  of  several  neurones  offer  caeteris  paribus 
higher  resistance  than  a  chain  of  fewer  neurones.  The  diagram 
is  therefore  in  accord  with  the  rule  that  the  reflex-chains  which 
conduct  to  parts  segmentally  distant  require  generally  intenser 
stimulation  to  excite  them  than  do  merely  local  arcs. 


IRRADIATION  157 


P 

^V      Short  and  long  reflexes.     For  many  purposes  of  description 
^■t  is  convenient  to  divide  reflexes  into  "  short "  and  "  long."  ^^ 
^B      The  cord  may,  in  its  relation  to  the  receptive  surface  and 
skeletal  musculature,  be  considered  divisible  into  right  and  left 
lateral  halves,  each  subdivisible  into  regions  of  neck  (cervical, 
^including  pinna),  fore  limb  (brachial),  trunk  (thoracic),  hind  Hmb 
crural),  and  tail  (caudal).     A  reflex  action  in  which  the  stimu- 
us  applied  to  a  receptive  area  in  one  of  the  above  regions 
vokes  a  reaction  in  the  musculature  of  another  of  the  regions 
s  conveniently  called  a  lofig  spinal  reflex.     A  reflex  reaction  in 
hich  the   muscular  reply  occurs  in  the   same  region  as  the 
pplication  of  the  stimulus  is  conveniently  called  a  short  spinal 
eflex.     Short  spinal  reflexes  are,  as  a  rule,  more   easily  and 
gularly  elicitable   than   are  long   spinal   reflexes.     It   might 
urther  be  convenient  to  allocate  hard-and-fast  boundaries   to 
hese  regions,  but  such  limits  would  of  necessity  be  artificial 
nd  arbitrary.     The  scope  of  the  delimitation  is  indicated  and 
ts  purpose  better  served  by  comparison  with  one  retina,  say  of 
he  bird,  the  one  lateral  half  of  the  skin  corresponding  with  one 
etina ;  one  optic  nerve  would  correspond  with  the  lateral  half  of 
he  spinal  cord  and  bulb.     Between  these  comparable  surfaces  a 
iflerence  exists,  in  that  the  receptive  field  of  the  skin,  unlike 
e  retinal,  has  instead  of  one  (or  two,  cf.  Kalischer)  ^^^  focal 
region  of  concentrated  responsiveness,  several  such  foci,  e.  g, 
the  relatively  highly  responsive  skin  at  the  apex  of  each  limb. 
As  the  retina  has  muscles  at  call,  so  also  the  skin.     The  close- 
ness of  nexus  between  a  retinal  point  and  the  visual  muscula- 
ture is  graduate  in  degree,  e,  g.  most  close  for  the  muscles  of  its 
own  bulbus,  next  for  those  of  the  contralateral,  then  for  the 
neck  muscles,  etc.   Similarly  there  are  degrees  of  nexal  closeness 
between  a  point  of  skin  and  the  related  musculature:  its  connec- 
tion is  most  close  with  muscles  of  its  own  limb,  next  with  those 
of  another  limb  or  other  region.     The  main  interest  of  direc- 
tion of  nervous   irradiation  per  se  —  apart   from  light  it   may 
incidently  cast  upon  the  integrative  work  of  the  nervous  system 
—  lies  in  its  elucidation  of  the  machinery  for  working  sentient 
surfaces.     That  the   skin   is   a  region   which  morphologically 


158  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect.  | 

considered  is  composed  of  a  segmental  series  seems  to  have  j 
been  allowed  greater  weight  in  the  estimation  of  its  receptive 
functions  than  is  fully  justified,  at  least  in  the  higher  vertebrata. 
That   its   segmental    innervation   demonstrably   limits   existing 
reflex  spinal  functions  in  the  mammal  has  not  been  shown.^^^ 

Rules  observed  in  the  spread  of  impulses  in  spinal  reflexes. 
Regarding  short  spinal  reflexes,  and  the  directions  taken  by  the 
examples  of  intraspinal  irradition  which  they  furnish,  it  is  possi 
ble  to  make  certain  general  statements.^^ 

I.  Broadly  speaking,  the  degree  of  reflex  spinal  intimacy  % 
between  affere^tt  and  efferent  spinal  roots  varies  directly  as  their  "\ 
segmental  proximity.  Thus  excitation  of  the  central  side  of  a  \ 
severed  thoracic  root,  e,  g,  seventh,  evokes  with  especial  ease  '[ 
contraction  of  muscles  or  parts  of  muscles  innervated  by  the  i 
corresponding  motor  roots,  and  next  easily  muscles  innervated  \ 
by  the  next  adjacent  motor  roots.  The  spread  of  short  spinal  : 
reflexes  in  many  instances  seems  to  be  rather  easier  tailward  \ 
than  headward.  This  may  be  related  with  the  oblique  correla-  \ 
tion  that  so  largely  holds  between  the  distribution  of  the  afferent  ^ 
root  in  the  skin  and  the  distribution  of  the  efferent  root  in  the  \ 
underlying  muscles. 

II.  Taken  generally,  for  each  afferent  root  there  exists  in  \ 
immediate  proximity  to   its  own  place  of  entrance  in  the  cord  \ 
{e.  g.  in  its  own  segment)  a  reflex  motor  path  of  as  low  a  thresh-  \ 
old   and   of  as   high   potency    as    any   open    to    it    anywhere,  \ 
Further,   in  response   to  excitation  even   approximately  mini-  • 
mal  in  intensity  a  single  afferent  root,  or  a  single  filament  of  ' 
a  single  root,  evokes  a  spinal  discharge  of  centrifugal  impulses  \ 
through  more   than   one   efferent   root,   i.  e,  the  discharge   is 
plurisegmental.     And  this  holds  especially  in  the  limb  regions. 
In  the  limb  region  the  nerve  root  is  therefore  a  morphological 
aggregate  of  nerve-fibres,  rather  than  a  functionally  determined 
assortment  of  impulse-paths.     The  view  that  the  efferent  spinal 
root  is  a  functional  assemblage  of  nerve-fibres  is  certainly  erro- 
neous.    The  formation  of  functional  collections  of  nerve  paths 
(peripheral  nerve-trunks)  out  of  morphological  collections  (nerve 
roots)  seems  to  be  the  meaning  of  the  limb-plexuses. 


I 


INHIBITION   REVEALS   IRRADIATION  159 


III.  Motor  mechanisms  for  the  skeletal  musculature  lying 
in  the  same  region  of  the  cord,  and  in  the  selfsame  spinal  seg- 
ment, exhibit  markedly  unequal  accessibilty  to  the  local  afferent 
channels  as  judged  by  pressor  effects.  For  example,  if  pressor 
jffects  only,  and  the  primary  phase  only,  of  the  reflex  movement 
be  considered,  the  flexors  of  the  homonymous  knee  and  the 
extensors  of  the  contralateral  are  in  many  animals  much  more 
accessible  than  the  extensors  of  the  homonymous  and  the 
flexors  of  the  contralateral.  Inasmuch  as  at  many  joints  the 
flexors  and  extensors  are  both  innervated  by  motor-fibres  con- 
tained in  one  and  the  same  efferent  root,  it  follows  that  the 
reflex  movement  obtained  by  excitation  of  an  afferent  root  in 
many  cases  is  quite  dissimilar  from  the  movement  obtained  by 
excitation  of  the  corresponding  efferent  root,  in  spite  of  the  rule 
of  segmental  proximity. 

It  is  necessary  to  insert  the  qualification  "  pressor "  before 
"effects"  ("reciprocal  innervation").  It  is  only  in  regard  to 
pressor  effect  that  the  above  statement  holds  for  such  contrasted 
neurones  as  those  of  "  extensors  "  and  "  flexors."  I  have  stated 
the  rule  in  this  way  because  more  in  conformity  with  the  oft- 
quoted  rule  of  spinal  reflexes  coming  to  us  from  Ludwig's  labor- 
atory, which  insisted  on  the  rarity  or  impossibility  of  obtaining 
hind-limb  extension  as  a  primary  homonymous  spinal  reflex. 
But  how  easy  and  direct  is  really  the  reflex  nexus  between  the 
receptive  surface  of  the  limb  and  its  extensor  muscles,  e.  g.  at 
knee,  is  shown  by  nothing  better  than  by  giving  a  small  dose  of 
strychnine.  That  alkaloid  has,  as  has  been  mentioned,  the 
property  of  converting  spinal  reflex  inhibition  into  excitation. 
The  same  stimulus  which  normally  reflexly  excites  the  knee- 
flexors  to  contraction  is  seen  after  the  strychnine  to  excite  the 
knee-extensors  to  contraction.  The  reflex  inhibition  of  the  ex- 
tensors which  was  previously  the  reflex-effect  is  more  difficult 
to  observe,  but  by  turning  it  into  excitation  the  facility  of  the 
reflex  nexus  with  the  extensors  is  found  to  be  as  close  as  with 
the  flexors.  Therefore,  in  the  rule  before  us,  if  inhibition  and 
excitation  are  both  —  as  they  should  be  —  counted  as  evidence 
of  the  reflex  nexus,  then  the  reflex  nexus  with  the  homony- 


i6o  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

mous  knee-extensors  and  with  the  crossed  knee-flexors,  is  as 
close  as  with  the  homonymous  knee-flexors  and  the  crossed 
knee-extensors. 

In  the  question,  therefore,  that  was  put  above.  How  is  it  that 
the  spread  of  a  reflex  reaction,  when  the  reflex  is  intensified, 
does  not  extend  to  all  parts,  as  it  is  said  to  do  in  strychnine 
poisoning  ?  there  are  two  different  things  involved.  It  does  not 
spread  to  some  parts  because,  as  argued  above,  an  additive 
synaptic  resistance  intervenes  across  the  potentially  conductive 
path.  An  instance  of  such  a  path  was  given.  But  the  absence 
of  apparent  irradiation  to  certain  others  is  for  a  different  reason. 
Keeping  to  the  flexion-reflex  as  elicited  by  unipolar  faradization 
of  the  plantar  skin  of  a  digit  as  illustration,  the  instance  of  the 
knee-extensor  may  be  taken.  However  intensely  the  stimula- 
tion may  be  pushed,  although  the  reflex  reaction  is  thereby 
more  and  more  intensified,  contraction  of  the  knee-extensor  does 
not  result,  —  but  for  a  wholly  different  reason  than  that  suggested 
for  the  absence  of  spread  of  the  reflex  from  the  leg  to  the  pinna. 
The  muscles  of  the  pinna,  in  my  experience,  do  not  at  all  easily 
become  involved  in  the  reaction;  but  the  extensor  muscle  of 
the  knee  is  really  involved  in  the  reaction  from  the  beginning, 
only  it  is  involved  in  a  way  that  escapes  observation  unless 
special  means  be  taken  to  reveal  it.  The  reflex-effect  upon 
it  takes  the  form  of  an  inhibition  of  the  efferent  path  just 
central  to  its  motor  neurone,  an  inhibitory  block,  which  in  pro- 
portion as  the  intensity  of  the  exciting  stimulus  of  the  reflex 
is  increased  simply  becomes  itself  the  more  intense.  There  is 
no  evidence  that  this  can  be  broken  down  and  converted  into 
excitation  by  merely  increasing  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus 
that  is  evoking  it.  On  the  other  hand,  as  shown  above,  strych- 
nine and  tetanus  toxin  convert  it  into  excitation,  and  that  is 
one  reason  why  strychnine  seems  to  increase  the  spread  of  re- 
flexes so  greatly;  but  in  this  case  the  increase  of  spread  which 
that  drug  appears  to  cause  is  really  merely  apparent.  The  reflex- 
effect  was  there  already,  but  had  another  form  of  expression. 

IV.    The  groups  of  motor  nerve-cells  contemporaneously  dis- 
charged by  spinal  reflex  action  innervate  synergic  and  not  antergic 


w 


IRRADIATION    IN   LONG   REFLEXES  i6i 


muscles.  This  is  the  reverse  of  the  view  that  since  Winslow 
and  Duchenne*^  has  been  common  doctrine  concerning  mus- 
cular co-ordination.  It  controverts  an  argument  adduced  for  the 
view  that  the  limb  movement  evoked  by  excitation  of  an  efferent 
root  represents  a  highly  co-ordinate  functional  synergism.^*  ^ 
The  spinal  reflex  in  its  intraspinal  irradiation  develops  a  com- 
bined movement  and  synthesizes  a  muscular  harmony. 

V.  It  follows  almost  as  a  corollary  from  this,  and  from  the 
rule  of  spatial  proximity  (p.  158),  that  the  spinal  reflex  movement 
elicitable  in  and  from  a7ty  one  spinal  region  will  exhibit  much 
uniformity  despite  considerable  variety  of  the  locus  of  incidence 
of  the  exciting  stimulus.  Approximately  the  same  movement, 
e.  g.  in  the  hind-limb  flexion  of  the  three  great  joints,  will  result, 
whatever  piece  of  the  limb  surface  be  irritated.  The  locus  of 
incidence  of  the  stimulus  will  only  influence  the  character  of  the 
general  movement  executed  by  the  limb  musculature,  in  so  far 
that  the  flexion  will  tend  to  predominantly  occur  at  that  joint 
the  flexor  muscles  of  which  are  innervated  by  motor  cells  seg- 
mentally  near  to  the  entrance  of  the  afferent  fibres  from  the 
particular  piece  of  skin  the  seat  of  application  of  the  stimulus. 
Another  way  of  expressing  this  rule  is  to  say  that  the  receptive 
field  of  a  **  type-reflex  "  is  usually  of  plurisegmental  cutaneous 
extent. 

Part  of  the  question  of  spatial  distribution  of  the  motor  dis- 
charge of  a  spinal  reflex  has  long  been  studied,  and  a  funda- 
mental contribution  to  knowledge  of  it  was  made  by  Pfluger.^ 
His  inductions  were  based  chiefly  upon  observations  on  the  frog 
and  on  the  records  of  clinical  cases  of  spinal  lesion  in  man. 
They  were  drawn  up  in  the  form  of  four  "  laws." 

It  was  regarding  the  course  of  irradiation  in  long  spinal 
reflexes,  namely  those  spinal  reflexes  that  initiated  from  07te  of 
the  above  mentioned  spinal  regions  spread  over  into  others  that 
Pfluger,2*  in  1853,  formulated  his  four  "  laws."  These  "Laws" 
have  for  many  years  been  widely  accepted.^^  They  are  stated 
as  follows :  — 

I.  The  law  of  homonymous  conduction  for  unilateral  re- 
flexes.    If  a  stimulus  applied  to  a  sensory  nerve  provokes  mus- 


i62  COMPOUND    REFLEXES  [Lect. 

cular  movements  solely  on  one  side  of  the  body,  that  move- 
ment occurs  under  all  circumstances  and  without  exception 
on  the  same  side  of  the  body  as  the  seat  of  application  of  the 
stimulus. 

If,  as  is  clear  from  the  context  in  the  original  paper,  by 
movement  on  the  same  side  is  meant  contraction  of  muscles  on 
the  same  side,  this  statement  does  not  in  reality  very  completely 
express  the  facts.^^  It  is  in  part  an  outcome  of  the  rule  of 
spatial  proximity,  but  certain  cases  which  conform  to  the  latter 
yet  offer  striking  exception  to  the  former ;  for  instance,  when  the 
skin  of  the  tail  is  stimulated  on  one  side  the  organ  is  very  fre- 
quently moved  towards  the  opposite,  and  this  in  a  great  number 
of  classes,  from  fish  to  mammal  inclusive. 

2.  The  law  of  bilateral  symmetry  of  the  reflex  action. 
When  the  change  produced  in  the  central  organ  by  excitation 
of  a  sensory  nerve  has  already  evoked  unilateral  reflex,  it,  if  it 
spreads  farther,  excites  in  the  contralateral  half  of  the  cord 
only  those  motor  mechanisms  which  are  symmetrical  with  those 
already  excited  in  the  homonymous  half  of  the  cord.  This 
statement,  although  true  of  a  number  of  instances,  fails  to  con- 
form with  fact  in  many,  even  perhaps  the  majority. 

The  important  cross-reflex  from  the  hind  limb  of  the  bird 
and  mammal  does  not  conform  to  it ;  so  similarly  with  the  fore 
limb.  The  asymmetry  of  the  crossed  reflexes  of  the  Hmbs  is 
important  because  probably  connected  with  the  fundamental 
co-ordination  of  muscles  for  progression.  Again,  the  wag- 
reflex  of  the  tail,  and  a  reflex  I  have  called  the  "torticollis 
reflex  "^^^  (cervical  region),  afford  important  exceptions  to 
the  "  law."  And  many  other  exceptions  can  be  found.  In  the 
spinal  rabbit,  on  the  other  hand,  and  less  often  in  the  dog,  the 
crossed  reflex  from  one  hind  limb  to  the  other  is  sometimes  not 
an  asymmetrical  movement,  but  a  symmetrical  one :  this  seems 
to  stand  in  obvious  relation  to  the  hopping  mode  of  progression 
of  the  animal. 

3.  The  law  of  unequal  intensity  of  bilateral  reflexes.  When 
the  excitation  of  a  sensory  nerve  elicits  reflex  action  involving 
both  halves  of  the  body,  and  the  action  is  unequal  on  the  two 


THE   REFLEX  FIGURE  163 

ides,  the  side  of  stronger  contractions  is  always  that  homony- 
mous with  the  seat  of  application  of  the  stimulus. 

This  statement  is  in  conformity  with  a  number  of  instances, 
he  following  are  examples.     When  bilateral  retraction  of  the 
bdomen  is  excited  from  the  skin  of  the  chest,  the  contralateral 
traction  is  much  the  less  marked :   in  the  bilateral  protraction 
f  the  "  whiskers "  (cat,  rabbit,  dog)  on  the  excitation  of  the 
in  of  the  face,  the  crossed  movement  is  the  less  ample.     An 
teresting   illustration,^^^*  ^^  because  it   involves  inhibitory  as 
ell  as  pressor  influence,  can  be  demonstrated  in  the  spinal  cat 
r  dog  thus:  — The  animal  resting  comfortably  on  its  back,  if 
ne  hind  paw  be  pressed  that  leg  will  be  flexed  at  hip,  knee, 
d  ankle,  in  accordance  with  the  rules  laid  down  on  p.  158,  and 
the  stimulus  be  strong,  or  the  reflex  excitabihty  good,  the 
fellow  hind  limb  will  be  extended.     If  instead  of  one  hind  paw 
both  hind  paws   be   pressed,  both  hind   limbs  are  simultane- 
ously flexed,  and  there  is  no  trace  of  extension  (Fig.  66,  Lect. 
yi.  p.  225).     The  homonymous  reflex  is  prepotent,  therefore, 
d  inhibits  the  crossed  reflex. 
But  there  are  also  a  number  of  exceptions  to  this  "  law," 
mong  others,  the  abduction  of  the  tail  from  the  side  stimulated 
ready  referred  to. 

4.  The  fourth  of  Pfliiger's  classical  "  laws  "  of  spinal  reflex 
tion  states  that  with  associated  spinal  reflex  centres  the  irradi- 
ion  spreads  more  easily  in  the  direction  toward  than  in  the 
irection  away  from  the  head. 

My  own  experience  in  the  mammal  is  far  from  completely 

cordant  with  this  statement:  in,  I  think,  the  majority  of  in- 

ances,  irradiation  has  spread  more  easily  down  than  up  the 

j.(j.i83, 205,251    j^  jg  g^sy  ^q  obtain  reflex  movements  of  the  limbs 

d  tail  by  excitation  of  the  skin   of  the  pinna,  whereas  the 

verse  is  rare.     To  elicit  by  excitation  of  the  hind  limb  a  move- 

ent  of  the  fore  limb,  is  more  difficult  than  by  excitation  of  the 

fore  limb  to  elicit  movement  of  the  hind  limb.     To  elicit  move- 

ent  of  the  tail  by  excitation  of  the  fore  limb  is  easier  than  to 

'move  the  fore  limb  by  excitation  of  the  tail.     The  irradiation 

has  in  my  experience  been  easier  across  the  cord  from  hind 


164 


COMPOUND   REFLEXES 
a  b 


[Lect. 


Figure  47.  — a.   Position  of  animal  after  transection  at  calamus  scriptorius. 

b.  Position  under  decerebrate  rigidity. 

c.  Change  of  attitude  from  b  evoked  by  stimulation  of  left  pinna. 

limb  to  hind  limb  than  from  hind  limb  to  fore  limb ;  but  it  is  often 
easier  down  the  cord  from  fore  limb  to  hind  limb  than  across 
from  fore  limb  to  fore  limb.  In  such  reflexes  also  as  the  "  shake  " 
reflex  (a  reflex  in  which  the  trunk  is  shaken,  as  when  a  dog  comes 
out  of  water),  which  implicate  the  trunk  more  than  the  limbs,  the 
radiation  is  away  from  the  head,  for  it  is  well  obtained  as  a  rump 
reflex  when  the  skin  of  the  shoulder  is  the  part  rubbed.  In  the 
"  scratch-reflex,"  too,  the  skin  stimulus  is  applied  far  headward 
of  the  region  of  the  muscular  contraction  evoked. 

These  so-called  **  laws  "  of  reflex  irradiation  were  so  generally 
accepted  as  to  obtain  a  doctrinal  eminence  which  they  hardly 
merit.  It  seems  here  less  profitable  to  attempt  adapting  them 
to  better  fit  the  observed  facts  than  to  briefly  describe  the 
salient  features  of  the  long  spinal  reflexes  as  exhibited  in  an 
ordinary  experiment  on  the  spinal  mammal. 

The  reflex  figure.  When  the  animal  is  supported  freely 
from  above,  with  its  spine  horizontal  and  the  limbs  pendant, 
a  point  that  early  strikes  the  observer  is  that  there  are  ten 
areas  whence  bulbo-spinal  reflexes  employing  skeletal  muscula- 
ture can  be  provoked  with  pre-eminent  facility.     These  areas 


\ 


THE  REFLEX  FIGURE 


i6s 


Figure  48.  —  a.   Position  under  decerebrate  rigidity. 

b.  Change  of  attitude  from  a  evoked  by  stimulation  of  left  fore  foot. 

c.  Change  of  attitude  from  a  evoked  by  stimulation  of  left  hind  foot. 

are  the  soles,  the  palms,  the  pinnae,  the  mouth,  the  snout,  and 
the  tail  and  cloacal  region.  It  is  significant  that  nine  of  these 
areas  are  those  which  possess  the  greatest  range  of  motility  if 
the  axis  of  the  animal  be  considered  fixed.  Stimulation  at  any 
one  of  these  areas  causes  a  particular  attitude  —  a  reflex  figure 
—  to  be  struck.  From  the  pinna  is  excited  movement  of  each 
limb,  the  neck,  thfe  tail,  and  the  trunk  (Fig.  47).  The  irradia- 
tion from  this  reflexigenous  area  usually  presents  the  following 
order:  (i)  Neck  and  homonymous  fore  limb,  (2)  homonymous 
hind  limb,  (3)  tail  and  trunk  on  both  sides,  (4)  contralateral 
hind  limb,  (5)  contralateral  fore  limb.  From  the  fore  foot  (Fig. 
48)  can  be  excited  besides  movements  in  the  fore  limb  itself, 
movements  in  the  other  limbs  and  tail.  The  facility  of  radia- 
tion is  usually  in  the  following  descending  series:  (i)  homony- 
mous hind  limb  and  the  tail,  (2)  crossed  hind  limb,  (3)  crossed 
fore  limb.  The  relative  facility  of  spread  of  the  reaction  to  the 
crossed  fore  limb  seems  subject  to  much  variation.  In  the  frog 
the  path  between  the  two  fore  limbs  is,  especially  in  the  breed- 
ing season,  very  open  and  facile.  In  the  cat  and  monkey  it 
seems  to  be  much  more  open  in  the  bulbo-spinal  than  in  the 
spinal  animal.     From  the  hind  foot   (Fig.  48)   the  frequency 


h 


i66  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

and  ease  of  irradiation  into  other  spinal  regions  usually  appears  to 
exist  in  the  following  order:  (i)  extension  of  crossed  hind  limb 
and  tail,  (2)  extension  of  homonymous  fore  limb,  (3)  flexion  of 
crossed  fore  limb.  The  facility  of  "  spread  "  from  one  lateral 
half  of  the  cord  to  the  other  is  very  dissimilar  at  different 
levels  of  the  cord.  It  is  particularly  easy  in  certain  parts  of 
the  tail  region.  Motor  mechanisms  which  are  yoked  together 
are  for  the  most  part,  as  with  the  flexion-extension  mechanisms 
of  the  hip  and  knee,  of  an  asymmetrical  kind.  In  the  hind-limb 
region  the  crossed  irradiation  is  also  fairly  free,  and  largely 
connects  asymmetrical  muscle-groups;  but  one  of  the  most 
facile  and  persistent  of  all  bilateral  reflexes  resulting  from  uni- 
lateral stimulation  is  the  adduction  of  both  thighs,  a  bilaterally 
symmetrical  movement. 

In  the  trunk  the  spread  across  the  median  plane  is  most  free 
for  skin  reflexes  excited  from  near  the  midline ;  this  is  seen  in 
the  venter  of  the  frog ;  the  yoking  is  of  bilaterally  symmetrical 
muscles.  To  excite  movement  of  one  fore  limb  from  the  other 
is  less  easy  than  to  excite  one  hind  limb  from  the  other,  at  least 
in  many  animals.  In  the  neck  region  irradiation  across  the 
median  sagittal  plane  is  fairly  easy,  and  the  yoking  connects  in 
large  part  asymmetrical  muscles. 

On  the  whole,  the  "  long  "  spinal  reflexes  are  more  variable 
and  less  validly  predictable  than  the  short.  They  vary  in  a 
series  of  experiments,  not  only  as  to  order  of  relative  facility  of 
direction  of  irradiation,  but  as  to  the  sense  of  the  movement 
elicited  at  the  joint,  whatever  it  may  be,  to  which  irradiation 
extends.  Not  unfrequently  a  region  to  which  the  reflex  usu- 
ally irradiates  is  altogether  omitted,  and  omitted  consistently 
throughout  the  whole  of  a  lengthy  experiment,  although 
the  spinal  region  in  question  has,  so  far  as  known,  suffered 
no  damage,  nor  indeed  been  directly  implicated  in  any  of  the 
procedure.  Thus,  excitation  of  the  skin  of  the  neck  or  pinna 
will  sometimes  spread  back  along  the  cord  and  produce  move- 
ment in  the  tail,  or  in  the  hind  limbs,  and  in  doing  so  pass  by 
the  fore  limbs  without  evoking  a  twitch  in  either  of  them.  The 
motor  mechanisms  of  the  fore  limb  thus  skipped  over  may  show 


r 


THE   REFLEX  FIGURE  167 


no  sign  when  examined  by  the  local  reflexes  of  being  less  amen- 
able than  usual. 

The  inconstancy  of  the  irradiation  as  to  the  kind  of  movement 
roduced,  e.  g.  whether  it  flex  or  extend  a  Umb,  is  diff'erent  in 
different  reflexes.  The  irradiations  from  the  "  drawing-up  reflex  " 
of  the  hind  foot  {i.  e,  the  flexion-reflex)  have  great  constancy ; 
the  irradiation  is  shown  in  Fig.  48 ;  to  the  figure  the  turning  of 
the  head  to  the  homonymous  side  may  be  added ;  the  irradia- 
tion from  the  fore-foot  reflex  is  less  regular ;  sometimes  flexion 
at  crossed  knee,  sometimes  extension.  The  variation  is  from 
experiment  to  experiment,  not  during  a  single  experiment. 

There  is  some  evidence  that  the  influence  exerted  on  a  com- 
mon path  by  one  and  the  same  afferent  arc  may  not  always  be 
of  the  same  kind.  It  is  true  that  the  regularity  with  which  the 
same  end-result  appears  and  reappears  in  observations  dealing 
with  certain  reflexes  is  very  great,  and  inclines  the  observer  to 
regard  the  reaction  of  the  reflex-arc  as  perfectly  constant.  But 
that  is  not  equally  clear  of  all  the  reflexes.  Instances  of  incon- 
stancy seem  to  occur  in  some  reflex-arcs,  and  these  suggest  the 
possibility  that  in  some  cases  one  and  the  same  aff"erent  arc  may 
exert  on  a  final  common  path  even  reverse  effects  at  different 
times ;  in  other  words,  under  different  conditions.  The  eff"ect  of 
stimuli  to  the  pinna  of  the  "  bulbo-spinal "  cat  seems  sometimes 
to  be  flexion  of  the  hind  limb,  sometimes  extension  of  that 
limb.  Stimulation  of  the  aff"erent  nerve  of  a  part  of  the  vasto- 
crureus  muscle  is  often  inhibition  of  the  rest  of  that  muscle, 
but  sometimes  not.  In  my  experience  these  results,  though 
variable  from  experiment  to  experiment,  do  not  vary  during 
the  same  experiment.  Again,  the  afferent  nerve,  stimulation 
of  which  excites  reflex  rise  of  arterial  pressure  under  curare, 
is  known  to  yield  reflex  fall  of  pressure  under  chloral.  It  must 
be  admitted  that  here  other  explanations  are  indeed  possi- 
ble, besides  the  supposition  that  the  kind  of  influence  excited 
by  the  aff'erent  arc  on  the  efferent  path  has  changed. 

Irradiation  of  a  reflex  attaches  itself  to  the  problem  of  the 
simultaneous  combination  of  reflexes.  It  does  so  because  it 
aff"ords  clear  evidence  that  by  irradiation  a  reflex  assumes  use 


i68  COMPOUND    REFLEXES  [Lect. 

of  a  number  of  final  common  paths  which  do  not  in  the  first 
instance  belong  to  it,  but  belong  in  the  first  instance  rather 
to  reflexes  arising  in  their  own  immediate  segmental  locality. 
From  them  a  '*  reflex  figure "  is  formed.  Thus,  by  irradi- 
ation, the  flexion-reflex  of  the  right  planta  causes  reflex- 
discharge  down  the  motor  nerve  of  the  cubital  extensor  (part 
of  triceps)  of  the  homonymous  fore  limb.  But  this  reflex 
motor  discharge  to  the  cubital  extensor  is  more  easily  excited 
by  stimulation  of  the  left  fore-paw.  Again  the  flexion-reflex 
of  the  right  planta,  if  strong,  will  irradiate  as  motor  discharge 
into  the  flexors  of  the  left  elbow,  but  the  reflex  motor  dis- 
charge into  the  flexors  of  the  left  elbow  is  much  more  easily 
obtained  by  stimulation  of  the  left  fore  paw ;  so  that  the  irra- 
diation welds  into  a  single  combined  reflex  efl"ects  belonging 
primarily,  as  it  were,  to  several  reflexes.  But  the  reflexes 
whose  efl*ects  are  thus  combined  are  always  reflexes  of  what 
was  termed  above  "  aUied  "  relation.  Thus,  if  a  stimulus  ex- 
citing the  flexion-reflex  from  the  right  planta  be  just  subliminal 
for  evoking  the  irradiation  to  the  homonymous  cubital  extensor 
and  a  stimulus  be  applied  to  the  left  fore  paw  of  an  intensity 
by  itself  just  subliminal  for  provoking  crossed  elbow-extension, 
the  two  stimuli  applied  simultaneously  mutually  facilitate  and 
the  reflex  of  the  right  fore  limb  results. 

Moreover,  it  seems  to  me  significant  that  the  irradiation 
extends  rather  per  saltum  than  gradatim.  As  the  flexion-reflex 
is  continued,  flexion  at  hip  (Fig.  45,  p.  153)  can  be  seen  to  add 
itself  almost  suddenly  to  flexion  already  in  progress  at  knee. 

Romanes ^2  writes  of  irradiation  in  Medusa  as  follows:  "It 
is  not  difficult  to  obtain  a  series  of  lithocysts  connected  in 
such  a  manner  that  the  resistance  off"ered  to  the  passage  of  the 
waves  by  a  certain  width  of  the  junction-tissue  is  such  as  just  to 
allow  the  residuum  of  the  contraction  wave  which  emanates  from 
one  lithocyst  to  reach  the  adjacent  lithocyst,  thus  causing  it 
to  originate  another  wave,  which  in  turn  is  just  able  to  pass  to 
the  next  lithocyst  in  the  series,  and  so  on,  each  lithocyst  acting 
in  turn  like  a  reinforcing  battery  to  the  passage  of  the  contrac- 
tion wave.     Now  this,  I  think,  sufficiently  explains  the  mech- 


i 


b 


V]  SIMULTANEOUS   COMBINATION  169 

anism  of  ganglionic  action  in  those  cases  where  one  or  more 
lithocysts  are  prepotent  over  the  others;  that  is  to  say,  the 
prepotent  lithocyst  first  originates  a  contraction  wave  which  is 
then  successively  reinforced  by  all  the  other  lithocysts  during 
its  passage  round  the  swimming-bell."  If  we  read  for  "  pre- 
potent lithocyst"  the  "exciting  external  stimulus"  of  the  arc 
primarily  stimulated  and  for  the  other  lithocysts  the  other  arcs 
to  which  the  excitement  of  the  one  primarily  stimulated  extends, 
it  seems  to  me  we  have  in  the  above  description  of  Aurelia 
aurita  a  description  that  applies  well  to  the  process  of  reflex 
irradiation  in  the  central  nervous  organ  of  vertebrates. 

It  may  be  objected  that  in  the  case  of  Medusa  the  wave  of 
contraction  is  reinforced  by,  on  reaching  the  lithocyst,  initiating 
through  that  a  new  reflex  which  reinforces  the  one  already  in 
progress;  whereas  in  the  spread  of  the  flexion-reflex  to  the 
reflex-arcs  of  the  fore  limb,  the  reaction  does  not  initiate  in 
these  latter  anything  that  can  be  called  a  new  reflex  because  the 
reaction  in  them  is  not  excited  through  their  local  receptors, 
the  normal  point  of  departure  for  their  reflexes.  That  is  a  dif- 
ference certainly,  and  a  significant  one.  But  it  does  not  vitiate 
the  analogy  from  the  point  of  view  under  consideration  now. 
In  Medusa  the  irradiation  of  the  reflex  is  in  its  propagation 
reinforced  at  the  certain  points  mentioned  by  the  reaction  in 
its  spread  exciting  a  new  neurone,  attached  to  its  path,  across  a 
definite  threshold  resistance.  In  Medusa  the  threshold  lies  in 
Romanes'  view  at  the  receptor  organ.  In  the  irradiation  of 
the  flexion-reflex  the  reaction  also  breaks  at  certain  points  into 
new  arcs  across  a  threshold  resistance,  and  once  over  the  thresh- 
old, propagates  itself  along  those  arcs,  as  evidenced  by  the 
movements  produced.  It  is  in  accordance  with  that  mode  of 
propagation  that  the  irradiation  of  the  reflex  appears  to  occur 
per  sal  turn  rather  than  gradatim  (Fig.  45).  Here  again  it  is 
noteworthy  that  the  places  of  reinforcement  in  the  spread 
of  the  reaction  which  are  peripheral  in  Medusa  are  central  in 
the  vertebrate;  in  other  words,  just  as  refractory  state,  inhibi- 
tion, interference,  etc.,  which  are  peripheral  in  Medusa  are 
central  in  the  vertebrate,  so  with  this  latter  instance  of  "rein- 


I/O  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

forcement."  The  reason  which  seemed  obvious  before,  appHes 
in  the  present  instance  also,  and  is  the  same  as  that  which  ex- 
plains the  centrality  of  the  central  nervous  system  itself  (Lect.  IX). 
It  is  not  only  when  the  spot  stimulated  is  a  receptor  that  the 
reflex  shows  itself  co-ordinate.  The  stimulation  of  the  central 
end  of  any  even  large  afferent  nerve-trunk,  or  even  the  central 
end  of  a  whole  spinal  afferent  root,  evokes  reflexly  a  movement 
that  is  co-ordinate.  This  result  is  familiar  and  commonplace, 
but  it  is  also  remarkable. 

For  stimulation  of  the  central  ends  of  that  vast  medley  of 
afferents,  from  different  sources  and  of  various  species,  collected 
together  in  one  afferent  spinal  root  {e.g.  eighth  cervical)  to  evoke 
no  inharmonious  confusion  of  various  reflexes,  such  as  the  com- 
ponent fibres  in  it  must,  taken  individually,  represent,  but  one 
allied  group  of  harmonious  reflexes,  is  a  result  that,  though 
regularly  obtainable,  is  surely  not  what  the  observer  might  have 
expected  would  occur.  Stimulation  of  the  central  end  of  the 
tibial  nerve  behind  the  ankle  causes  flexion  at  knee,  hip,  and 
ankle,  in  which  the  normal  inhibitions  of  extensor  muscles  ac- 
company the  contractions  of  the  flexor  muscles.  Yet  in  that 
nerve  are  included  the  afferent  fibres  from  the  planta  which 
evolve  the  powerful  extension-reflex,  the  extensor-thrust.  How 
is  it  that  this  reflex  does  not  appear  conjoined  with  the  flexion- 
reflex  produced  by  the  other  afferent  fibres  in  the  posterior  root 
or  tibial  nerve?  The  principle  of  interference  of  antagonistic 
reflexes  central  to  the  mouth  of  the  common  path  on  which  both 
embouch  precludes  such  confusion  of  reflexes.  Such  simulta- 
neous combination  of  flexion  and  extension  reflexes  would  be 
inco-ordination.  The  formation  of  a  common  path  from  tribu- 
tary paths  is  a  mechanism  ensuring  co-ordination  against  that. 
The  stimulation  of  the  nerve  containing  admixed  receptive  paths 
of  different  and  antagonistic  reflexes  excites  reflexly  through 
the  central  organ  an  effect  in  the  skeletal  musculature  which  is 
co-ordinate  and  synergic.  It  is  as  though  a  solvent  were  simul- 
taneously supersaturated  with  two  crystalloids,  and  as  though 
when  a  pair  of  these  crystals  were  simultaneously  dropped  into 
the  solution  the  crystallization  out  took  place  of  one  salt  or  of 


REFLEX   FIGURE   INVOLVES   INHIBITION       171 

he  other,  but  not  of  both  together.  What  happens  resembles 
what  occurs  when  there  is  presented  to  the  eye  one  of  the  plane 
figures  suggesting  visual  perspective,  but  equivocally  in  either 
of  the  two  ways ;  the  whole  of  the  beaker  or  of  the  flight  of 
steps  appears  set  in  one  way  or  in  the  other,  never  partly  in 
one  way,  partly  in  the  other.  And  this  is  a  really  germane 
analogy. 

The  mode  of  preclusion  of  the  antagonistic  reflexes  seems 
so  closely  akin  to  the  process  which  occurs  when  one  reflex  in 
its  supervention  on  another  dispossesses  an  antagonistic  reflex 
from  the  common  path  that  its  discussion  may  be  deferred  until 
treating  of  the  co-ordination  of  successive  reflexes.  But  it  is 
obvious  that  in  the  irradiation  of  a  reflex  so  as  to  produce  a 
combined  movement  of  remote  parts  we  have  really  a  syn- 
thesis of  simultaneous  reflexes.  The  parts  of  the  reflex  finding 
simultaneous  expression  in  the  eff'erent  paths  of  other  reflexes 
are  combined  by  a  process  which  tends  to  exclude  the  antago- 
nistic reflex  for  each  component  part.  It  is  obvious  that  while 
"  allied  "  reflexes  can  be  compounded  together  both  in  simulta- 
neous and  successive  combination,  antagonistic  reflexes  can  be 
combined  only  in  successive  combination. 

The  collection  of  fibres  in  a  motor  spinal  root  does  not  repre- 
sent a  "reflex  figure."  The  supporters  of  the  view  that  the 
motor-fibres  gathered  together  in  a  motor  spinal  root  form  a 
collection  assorted  so  as  to  represent  the  fibres  that  normally 
are  excited  together  in  willed  and  other  actions  adduce  the  fact 
that  antagonistic  muscles  are  together  thrown  into  contraction 
on  exciting  this  or  that  spinal  motor  nerve  root  supplying  a  limb 
—  e.  g.,  the  arm.  This  fact,  as  I  pointed  out  some  years  ^^  ago, 
is  in  reality  one  of  the  clearest  evidences  that  their  view  is 
erroneous,  because  most  commonly  in  normal  movements  the 
antagonistic  muscles,  far  from  being  thrown  into  contraction 
together,  are  reciprocally  innervated,  one  antagonist  being  made 
to  contract  and  the  other  to  relax.  Further,  far  from  a  normal 
action  ever  throwing  into  activity  all  the  motor-fibres  of  a 
single  motor  root,  still  less  using  that  one  root  thus  solely 
without  other  roots,  in  reality  the  evidence  is  that  in  all  normal 


1/2  COMPOUND    REFLEXES  [Lect. 

actions,  reflex  or  voluntary,  especially  in  the  limb  regions,  the 
centrifugal  discharge  to  the  muscles  takes  place  through  scattered 
motor-fibres  contained  in  several  roots,  even  when  the  action  pro- 
voked and  the  movement  effected  are  weak.  Outside  the  limb 
region,  those  who  argue  that  the  aggregation  of  motor-fibres  in 
each  efferent  spinal  root  represents  some  definite  synergic  con- 
traction of  muscles  for  a  co-ordinate  movement  must  disregard 
the  observation  of  Newell  Martin,  and  HartwelF^  that  in  the 
normal  breathing  of  the  dog  the  action  that  goes  forward  in  the 
internal  and  external  intercostal  muscles  is  alternating  in  them. 
One  relaxes  as  the  other  contracts ;  yet  both  the  external  and 
the  internal  intercostal  muscle  of  the  space  receive  their  motor 
supply  from  one  and  the  same  motor  spinal  root. 

Those  who  hold  the  view  that  the  assortment  of  the  fibres 
of  the  motor  root  is  functional,  state  that  the  movements  which 
result  from  stimulation  of  these  individual  roots  in  the  brachial 
region  are  not  mere  contractions  more  or  less  strong  of  various 
muscles,  but  are  a  highly  co-ordinated  functional  synergy  in 
each  case.  To  this  one  may  reply  that  the  mere  superficial 
resemblance  of  the  position  assumed  by  the  limb,  to  one 
of  the  manifold  positions  assumed  by  it  in  the  normal  activity, 
is  a  slender  analogy.  The  hind  limbs  of  a  frog,  when  it  tries 
to  climb  the  side  of  the  bell-jar  which  confines  it,  assume  an 
attitude  of  extreme  extension,  in  outward  semblance  like  that 
of  a  strychnine  cramp,  or  that  due  to  excitation  of  the  eighth 
root ;  but  is  it  permissible  from  that  resemblance  to  argue  that 
excitation  of  the  eighth  root  produces  a  co-ordinate  move- 
ment of  the  limb?  The  same  analogy  would  argue  that  the 
strychnic  cramp  is  also  a  co-ordinate  movement  of  the  limb, 
whereas  it  is  definitely  known  to  be  inco-ordinate. 

Myself  I  have  not  been  struck  by  resemblance  between  the 
movement  produced  by  excitation  of  the  motor  spinal  root  of  a 
limb-plexus  and  the  co-ordinate  normal  movements  of  the  limb. 
It  may  be  urged  that  in  order  to  obtain  the  resemblance  the 
excitation  employed  must  be  strong,  so  as  to  bring  into  full 
action  every  component  of  the  complex  entity  of  the  root. 
When  I  have  done  this  the  resulting  movement  has  seemed  to 


r 


]        FIGURE  NOT   GIVEN  BY  MOTOR  ROOT       173 

me,  e.  g.  in  the  case  of  the  sixth  subthoracic  root  of  the  monkey, 
like  a  strychnine  cramp  rather  than  a  movement  of  co-ordinate 
adjustment.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  it  be  urged  that  minimal 
excitation  must  be  used,  I  have  not  been  able  to  obtain  in  that 
way  any  more  obvious  relation  to  a  co-ordinate  movement.  For 
instance,  in  the  lumbo-spinal  region  of  the  monkey,  excitation 
when  just  effective  induces  through  the  ninth  subthoracic  motor 
root  abduction  of  the  tail  and  flexion  of  the  toes  without  any 
movement  elsewhere.  Similar  excitation  of  the  eighth  motor 
root  induces  reflexion  of  minimus  and  hallux  without  the  inter- 
vening digits,  not  infrequently  accompanied  by  pursing  of  the 
anus.  Such  combinations  strike  the  observer  as  bizarre  and  give 
little  suggestion  of  the  bringing  into  play  of  a  highly  co-ordinate 
functional  synergy. 

On  the  view  that  the  compound  muscular  contraction 
obtained  by  excitation  of  one  whole  motor  root  is  highly  co- 
ordinated and  due  to  a  group  of  contractions  combined  in 
accordance  with  some  plan  for  a  functional  result,  it  might  be 
expected  that  the  severance  of  one  such  motor  root  in  a  limb 
region  would  result  in  loss  of  some  particular  co-ordinate  move- 
ment, and  that  the  disappearance  of  that  movement  might  be 
fairly  clearly  detectible.  I  was  unable  to  detect  such  a  result 
and  saw  no  evidence  in  support  of  its  existence.  The  severance 
of  a  single  motor  root  seemed  to  produce  not  the  complete  loss 
of  any  one  particular  movement,  but  a  weakened  condition  of 
many  movements.  Even  when  two  of  the  motor  spinal  roots 
were  cut  the  effect  on  the  movements  of  the  limb  was  rather 
weakness  of  movement  than  inco-ordination  of  movement.  When 
the  number  of  consecutive  nerves  cut  was  more  than  two  there 
appeared  limitation  of  the  range  of  movement  by  loss  in  some 
particular  direction.  I  inferred  from  my  experiments  that  the 
mechanism  for  specific  movement  of  each  part  of  a  limb  {e.  g. 
a  digit)  is  so  placed  in  the  cord  that  the  efferent  fibres  debouch- 
ing from  it  into  the  motor  roots  pass  via  many  root  filaments 
and  via  at  least  two,  usually  more,  spinal  roots.  Thus  there  is\ 
not  in  any  one  motor  root  filament,  nor  even  in  any  one  motor 
root,  a  perfect  representation  of  any  one  movement,  but  only  an 


174  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

imperfect  representation  of  several  adjacent  local  movements, 
though  not  for  each  equally  imperfect. 

Against  the  view  that  the  aggregation  of  efferent  fibres  in  a 
motor  spinal  root  represents  a  functionally  co-operative  collection 
is  the  fact  that  between  them  there  may  intervene  a  high  resist- 
ance; that  is  to  say,  afferent  impulses  that  easily  throw  some 
of  them  into  action  have  great  difficulty  in  throwing  others  of 
them  into  action.  Thus,  the  ninth  subthoracic  motor  root  of  the 
monkey  sometimes  contains  efferent  fibres  to  the  urinary  bladder 
and  to  the  muscles  of  the  leg.  It  is  easy  to  obtain  a  reflex  on 
the  latter  through  the  afferent  root  of  the  ninth,  but  relatively 
difficult  to  obtain  a  reflex  upon  the  bladder.  The  synergetic 
view  of  the  character  of  the  collection  of  fibres  in  the  spinal 
motor  root  presupposes  or  infers  that  mere  spatial  juxtaposi- 
tion possesses  curiously  high  value  for  sf)inal  co-ordination  ;  but 
are  the  separate  spinal  and  bulbar  elements  of  the  respiratory 
centre  less  perfectly  associated  in  function  because  in  the  neural 
axis  they  are  placed  apart?  Likeness  of  quality  rather  than 
proximity  in  space  insures  the  harmony  of  their  reactions.  I 
conclude,  therefore,  that  the  collection  of  fibres  in  a  spinal 
motor  root  is  not  a  functional  collection  in  the  sense  that  it  is 
representative  of  any  co-ordination.^^ 

The  receptive  field  of  a  reflex  does  not  conform  with  the  field 
of  distribution  of  an  afferent  spinal  root.  Similarly  with  the 
afferent  root.  The  distribution  in  the  skin  of  any  afferent  root 
does  not  correspond  with  the  receptive  field  of  any  cutaneous 
reflex.  The  skin-field  of  the  scratch-reflex  is  made  up  of  parts 
of  the  skin-fields  of  many  adjacent  spinal  roots  (compare  Fig. 
49  with  Fig.  39,  p.  121).  The  skin-field  of  the  flexion-reflex 
of  the  hind  limb  similarly;  that  of  the  fore  limb  similarly. 
Even  the  relatively  limited  receptive  field  ^^^  of  the  extensor- 
thrust  is  a  patch  into  which  parts  of  the  fields  of  at  least  two 
spinal  roots  enter.  Nor  do  the  limits  of  the  receptive  fields  of 
cutaneous  reflexes  respect  the  limits  of  spinal  root  skin-fields. 
Again,  the  afferent  nerve  of  the  extensor  cruris  muscle  evokes 
the  same  reflex  as  does  that  of  the  flexor  muscle,  yet  the  two 
belong  to  wholly  different  spinal  nerves. 


to 


V]  REINFORCEMENT  175 


Reinforcement.  The  overflow  of  reflex  action  into  channels 
belonging  primarily  to  other  reflex-arcs  than  that  under  stimula- 
tion leads  to  the  production  by  the  single  stimulus  of  a  wide, 
compound  reflex  which  is  tantamount  in  effect  to  a  simultaneous 
combination  of  several  allied  reflexes. 

When  in  the  spinal  animal  the  one  fore  foot  is  stimulated, 
flexion  of  the  hind  leg  of  the  crossed  side  is  often  obtained. 
Stimulation  of  that  hind  foot  itself  also  causes  a  like  reflex  of 
that  limb.  When  these  two  are  concurrently  stimulated,  the 
flexion  movement  is  obtained  more  easily  than  from  either 
singly.  These  widely  separate  reflex-arcs  therefore  reinforce 
one  another  in  their  action  on  the  final  common  paths  they 
possess  in  common.  Similarly  with  certain  reflex-arcs  arising 
from  the  skin  of  the  pinna  of  the  crossed  ear.  In  them  excita- 
tion reinforces  that  of  the  just  mentioned  arcs  from  the  fore  foot 
and  opposite  hind  foot. 

This  reinforcement  is  significant  of  the  solidarity  of  the  whole 
spinal  mechanism ;  but  significant  of  more  extensive  solidarity 
still  are  results  observed  by  Exner.^»^*^  A  sound  conveyed 
to  the  ear  of  a  chloralized  rabbit,  he  found,  increased  the  am- 
plitude of  a  reflex  movement  of  the  foot,  induced  by  the  stimulus 
applied  to  the  foot  a  moment  later.  Sternberg  has  studied 
similar  summation  of  reflexes. ^^^  The  same  significance  proba- 
bly attaches  to  the  influence  of  various  precurrent  stimuli  on 
the  knee-jerk  in  man,  studied  by  Jendrassik,^^^  Mitchell  and 
Lewis,^^^  Lombard,^^^  and  by  Bowditch  and  Warren.^  In  these 
cases  of  course  cerebral  as  well  as  subcerebral  arcs  were  in 
action.  And  in  regard  to  these,  we  have  the  observations  by 
Bubnoff  and  Heidenhain,^  and  by  Exner,^  in  the  narcotized 
dog  and  rabbit.  In  their  experiments  gentle  stimuli  to  the  skin 
of  a  limb  exerted  a  reinforcing  influence  on  closely  following 
stimuli  applied  to  the  hmb  region  of  the  cortex  of  the  brain. 
Exner's  observations  proved  that  minimal  electrical  stimuli 
applied  near  together  in  point  of  time  to  the  fore-limb  region 
of  the  rabbit's  cortex  and  to  the  skin  of  the  crossed  foot,  ex- 
erted a  facilitating  influence,  "  bahnung,"  on  each  other.  He 
points  out  that  this  reinforcement  occurs  when  the  cortex  itself 


176  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect, 

has  been  removed,  and  the  stimulation  of  the  brain  is  applied 
direct  to  the  underlying  white  matter.  He  argues,  therefore, 
that  the  seat  of  production  of  the  facilitation  lies  in  the  spinal 
centres.  With  that  view,  the  argument  followed  here  is  in  com- 
plete accord. 

The  co-ordination,  in  some  of  the  instances  taken,  has  cov- 
ered but  one  limb  or  a  pair  of  Hmbs.  But  the  same  principle 
extended  to  the  reactions  of  the  great  arcs  arising  in  the 
projicient  receptor  organs  of  the  head,  e.  g.  the  eye,  that  deal 
with  wide  tracts  of  musculature  as  a  whole,  involves  further- 
reaching  co-ordination.  The  singleness  of  action  from  moment 
to  moment  thus  assured  is  a  keystone  in  the  construction  of  the 
individual  whose  unity  it  is  the  specific  office  of  the  nervous 
system  to  perfect.  And  in  the  instance  taken,  namely,  concur- 
rent stimulation  of  the  one  fore  foot  and  the  crossed  hind  foot, 
the  co-ordination  can  be  easily  traced  further ;  the  crossed  fore 
foot  is  extended  at  elbow  and  retracted  at  shoulder  under  the 
combination  of  the  two  stimuli,  and  the  homonymous  hind  limb 
is  extended  at  knee  and  hip.  We  might  also  add  to  these 
movements  others,  also  caused  by  the  same  stimulus,  of  the 
eyeballs,  the  lips,  the  larynx,  and  the  arterial  wall  of  the 
splanchnic  area.  But  these  would  not  for  the  present  purpose 
emphasize  the  main  point  further. 

As  remarked  above,  it  is  not  usual  for  the  organism  to  be 
exposed  to  the  action  of  only  one  stimulus  at  a  time.  It  is 
more  usual  for  the  organism  to  be  acted  on  by  many  stimuli 

Figure  49  (opposite).  — The  skin-fields  of  the  afferent  spinal  roots  of  the  monkey  (Macacus 
rhesus),  showing  their  general  arrangement  in  the  trunk  and  hind  limb.  On  the  right  side  only 
the  posterior  limit  of  each  field  is  shown,  on  the  left  side  only  the  anterior  limit.  The  fields 
were  observed  by  the  method  of  "  remaining  aesthesia."  After  determination  of  the  limits 
of  the  field  for  a  spinal  root  in  a  number  of  individuals  the  mean  of  the  observations  for  that 
root  was  transferred  to  a  plaster  cast  of  Macacus  rhesus,  and  the  lines  thus  gradually  built 
up  on  the  model.  The  dotted  line  extending  from  the  mid-dorsum  outward  along  the 
dorsal  aspect  of  the  thigh  is  the  "  dorsal  line  "  of  the  hind  limb,  and  to  it  the  fields  of 
the  sensory  roots  distributed  to  the  skin  of  the  limb  behave  as  do  those  of  the  skin-fields  of 
the  trunk  to  the  mid-dorsal  line  of  the  body.  It  will  be  noticed  that  the  boundaries  of  the 
spinal  root-fields  neither  in  the  limb  nor  in  the  trunk  conform  with  the  limits  of  the  "  re- 
ceptive-fields "  of  cutaneous  reflexes.  The  cutaneous  fields  of  the  "scratch-reflex,"  the 
"flexion-reflex,"  the  "  extensor-thrust,"  are  areas  which  in  nowise  fit  in  with  the  pattern 
of  the  cutaneous  fields  of  the  afferent  spinal  roots.  Compare  this  figure  with  Figure  13  A, 
Lecture  II,  p.  46. 


V] 


REINFORCEMENT 


177 


178  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

concurrently,  and  to  be  driven  reflexly  by  some  group  of  stimuli 
which  is  at  any  particular  moment  prepotent  in  action  on  it. 
Such  a  group  often  consists  of  some  one  pre-eminent  stimulus 
with  others  of  harmonious  relation  reinforcing  it,  forming  with 
it  a  constellation  of  stimuli,  that,  in  succession  of  time,  will 
give  way  to  another  constellation  which  will  in  its  turn  become 
prepotent. 

The  concurrent  stimuli  keep  a  number  of  arcs  in  active 
touch  with  their  final  paths,  and  a  number  of  other  arcs  out  of 
active  touch  with  the  final  paths  belonging  to  them.  In  the 
particular  instance  taken,  they  keep  arcs  of  one  fore  limb  and 
one  hind  limb  in  action  upon  final  common  paths  of  flexion  of 
those  limbs,  upon  final  common  paths  of  extension  in  the  diago- 
nal pair  of  limbs,  and  upon  final  common  paths  of  flexion  of  the 
neck.  And  the  concurrent  stimuli  simultaneously  check  other 
arcs  from  getting  into  active  touch  with  the  final  common  paths 
of  extension  of  one  fore  limb  and  hind  limb,  namely,  those  of 
the  seat  of  stimulations,  and  of  flexion  in  the  opposite  fore  limb 
and  hind  limb,  and  of  retraction  of  the  neck.  Further,  these 
reactions  certainly  receive  reinforcement  through  the  arcs  of  re- 
ceptors in  the  muscles  and  through  arcs  arising  in  the  receptors 
of  the  otic  labyrinth.  An  instance  of  reinforcement  of  this 
very  kind  from  muscular  receptors  we  have  already  given 
(Lecture  IV,  p.  131). 
n  Thus  at  any  single  phase  of  the  creature's  reaction,  a  simul- 
taneous combination  of  reflexes  is  in  existence.  In  this  combi- 
nation the  positive  element,  namely,  the  final  common  paths 
(motor  neurone  groups)  in  active  discharge,  exhibits  a  harmo- 
nious discharge  directed  by  the  dominant  reflex-arc,  and  rein- 
forced by  a  number  of  arcs  in  alliance  with  it.  The  dominant 
reflex-arc  in  the  instance  taken  is  that  from  the  noci-ceptors  of  the 
right  hind  foot.  The  reinforcing  arcs  are  at  this  phase  of  the  re- 
action certain  direct  extension  arcs,  certain  proprio-ceptive  arcs, 
and  certain  labyrinthine  arcs.  But  there  is  also  a  negative  ele- 
ment in  this  simultaneous  combination  of  reflexes.  The  reflex 
not  only  takes  possession  of  certain  final  common  paths  and  dis- 
charge nervous  impulses  down  them,  but  it  takes  possession  of 


SIMULTANEOUS   COMBINATION  179 

e  final  common  path  whose  muscles  would  oppose  those  into 
which  it  is  discharging  impulses,  and  checks  their  nervous  dis- 
charge responsive  to  other  reflexes.  This  negative  part  of 
the  field  of  influence  of  the  reflex  is  more  difficult  to  see,  but 
it  is  as  important  as  the  positive,  to  which  it  is  indeed  comple- 
mental.  Therefore  it  is  that  the  reflex  initiated  by  one  group  of 
receptors  while  in  progress  excludes  in  various  directions  the  re- 
flexes of  other  receptors,  although  these  latter  may  be  being 
stimulated.  In  this  way  the  motor  paths  at  any  moment  accord 
in  a  united  pattern  for  harmonious  synergy,  co-operating  for  one 
eff"ect. 

The  notion,  therefore,  that  we  arrive  at  of  such  a  motor 
reflex  reaction  is  that  it  is  referable  to  a  constellation  of  con- 
gruous stimuli  of  which  one  is  prepotent,  and  that  the  reac- 
tion taken  in  its  totality  gives  the  nervous  intercommunications 
of  the  central  organ  a  certain  pattern,  which  pattern  may 
ramify  through  a  great  extent  of  the  central  organ.  This  reac- 
tion has  its  positive  side  traceable  as  active  discharge  from  a 
number  of  end-points  of  the  nervous  network,  and  its  negative 
side  symmetrically  opposed  to  its  positive  and  traceable  con- 
versely by  check,  depression,  or  absence  of  nervous  discharge. 
Even  in  extensive  reflexes  of  the  bulbo-spinal  animal  it  is  prob- 
able that  though  great  fields  of  the  nervous  centres  are  involved 
in  the  reaction  at  any  one  time,  large  parts  are  still  left  outside 
the  reaction.  This  part  of  the  neural  network  would  therefore 
be  indifferent  to  that  particular  reaction.  That  amounts  to  say- 
ing that  it  is  open  during  the  reaction  to  be  thrown  into  activity 
by  some  concurrent  and  distinct  other  reaction.  But  this 
possible  neutrality  and  discreteness  of  reflex  reaction  and  its 
fields  is  probably  far  less  in  the  intact  higher  vertebrate  than  in 
the  lower  or  in  the  mutilated  higher  vertebrate.^^  In  the  pres- 
ence of  the  brain  the  knitting  together  of  the  whole  nervous 
network  is  probably  much  greater  than  in  its  absence. 

A  question  arises  concerning  the  simultaneous  combination 
of  reflexes  which  is  closely  related  to  that  regarding  the  grad- 
ing of  intensity  of  a  reflex. 

Some  reflexes  exhibit  many  grades  of  intensity  under  grad- 


i8o  COMPOUND   REFLEXES  [Lect. 

■1; 
ing  of  intensity  of  stimulus.     The  flexion-reflex  is  an  instance.  | 

There  as  the  skin  stimulus  is  increased  the  height  to  which  I 
the  foot  is  flexed  is  increased.  But  it  seems  obvious  that  : 
such  an  effect  is  not  to  be  expected  in  all  reflexes.  Where,  ; 
as,  for  instance,  in  the  scratch-reflex,  the  foot  has,  in  response  ; 
to  irritation  at  a  certain  spot,  to  be  moved  to  that  spot,  it  I 
would  defeat  the  use  of  the  reflex  for  a  strong  stimulus  to  flex  * 
the  limb  further,  so  as  to  carry  it  beyond  the  spot  required.  \ 
And  we  see  that  as  the  scratch-reflex  is  increased  in  intensity  \ 
the  increase  does  not  appreciably  increase  the  amount  of  tonic 
flexion  exhibited  by  the  reflex,  but  spends  itself  in  increasing  • 
the  clonic  beat  of  the  reflex,  which  still  oscillates  about  the  ; 
same  median  position.  When  the  scratch-reflex  is  elicited  by 
simultaneous  combination  of  two  reflexes  initiated  from  spots  ■ 
near  together  in  the  receptive  field  the  tonic  flexion  under-  1 
lying  either  reflex  does  not  in  my  experience  appear  to  sum  | 
with  that  of  the  other;  the  summation  that  appears  seems  ^ 
confined  to  the  more  vigorous  clonic  beating  of  the  combined 
reflex.  ^! 

It  seems  therefore  likely  that  in  the  simultaneous  combina- 
tion of  reflexes  the  reinforcement  that  goes  on,  although  it  is  \ 
sometimes  expressed  as  greater  amplitude  of  contraction,  is  not  ■ 
necessarily  so  expressed  in  all  cases.  Just  as  various  type-  ; 
reflexes  exhibit  extreme  individuality  of  time-relations,  intensity  ; 
grading,  etc.,  so  also  they  exhibit  in  their  mode  of  simultaneous  \ 
combination  individual  differences  of  high  degree. 


IVlj  SUCCESSIVE   COMBINATION  i8i 

■  LECTURE    VI 

COMPOUND   REFLEXES:    SUCCESSIVE   COMBINATION. 

I        Argument:  Co-ordination  of  reflex  sequences.     Chain-reflexes  (Loeb). 

i  Overlapping  of  successive  stimuli  in  time.  The  sequence  of  allied 
reflexes.  Spread  of  bahnung,  "  immediate  induction."  Sequence  of 
antagonistic  reflexes.  The  role  of  inhibition  in  this  transition.  Views 
of  the  nature  of  inhibition :  Rosenthal,  Wundt,  E.  Hering,  Gaskell, 
Verworn,  J.  S.  Macdonald.  The  "  interference  "  of  reflexes.  "  Alter- 
nating reflexes."  VV.  Macdougall's  view  of  "drainage  of  energy." 
"Compensatory  reflexes."  Factors  determining  the  issue  of  the 
competition  between  antagonistic  reflexes.  "  Successive  induction." 
Rebound-effects  in  spinal  reactions ;  tend  to  restore  reflex  equilibrium. 
Fatigue  in  reflexes.  Relative  high  resistance  to  fatigue  possessed 
by  the  final  common  path,  /.  e.  motor  neurone.  Intensity  of  reac- 
tion a  decisive  factor  in  the  competition  of  afferent  arcs  for  posses- 
;-  sion  of  the  final  common  path.     Noci-ceptive  nerves.     Prepotency 

I^K  of  reflexes  generated  by  receptors  that  considered  as  sense  organs 
I^H  initiate  sensations  with  strong  affective  tone.  Resistance  of  tonic 
I^B  reflexes  to  fatigue.  All  these  factors  render  the  conductive  pattern 
^H      of  the  central  nervous  system  mutable  between  certain  limits. 

We  considered  last  the  co-ordination  of  reflexes  in  simultane- 
ous combination.  We  now  turn  to  sequence  of  reflexes.  Re- 
flexes are  seen  to  follow  one  another  in  consecutive  combination. 
And  in  this  chaining  together  of  successive  reflexes  in  differ- 
ent instances  different  kinds  of  processes  seem  traceable.  Of 
these,  one  consists  in  the  reaction  to  one  external  stimulus 
bringing  about  an  application  of  an  external  stimulus  for  a 
second  reflex.  The  dart-reflex  of  the  frog's  tongue  provoked 
by  the  seen  fly  provides,  if  successful,  the  stimulus  (contact 
with  the  mucosa  of  the  mouth)  which  provokes  closure  of 
the  mouth,  and  this  probably  insures  the  stimulus  for  the 
ensuing  deglutition,  and  so  on.  Exner  has  dealt  with  this  kind 
of  chaining  together  of  reflexes  by  one  stimulus  leading  to 
another  in  his  "  Entwurf  einer  physiologischen  Erklarung  psy- 
chischer  Erscheinungen."     Loeb  has  illustrated  it  luminously  in 


1 82  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

his  "  Physiology  of  the  Brain."  He  calls  sequence  of  reflexes 
proceeding  by  this  process  from  one  segmental  reflex  to  another 
"  chain-reflexes "  (Ketten-reflexe).  Mosso,^^  Kronecker  and 
Meltzer,  ^' ^' ^^^  Chauveau,^^^  and  Zwaardemaker,  2^^' '^^'^  j^^yg 
traced  such  reflexes  analytically  in  deglutition. 

Mosso^  showed  that  in  the  oesophageal  stage  of  deglutition 
each  reflex  in  a  part  of  the  tube  above  is  immediately  succeeded 
by  a  reflex  ensuing  in  the  adjoining  part  below,  and  yet  in  this 
sequence  the  distant  bulbar  centre  is  itself  concerned,  since  the 
sequence  ceases  if  the  branches  of  the  vagus  containing  the 
nerve  paths  to  and  from  that  centre  be  severed.  Biedermann 
has  recently  demonstrated  a  very  similar  procession  of  reflexes 
in  the  crawling  of  the  earthworm,  and  there  again  the  sequence 
involves  reflexes  conducted  through  the  central  nervous  system. 
It  appears  that  the  action  of  each  preceding  segment  provides 
a  stimulus  for  the  reflex  act  of  the  next  succeeding  segment. 

Orderly  sequence  of  movement  characterizes  the  outward 
behaviour  of  animals.  Not  least  so  where,  as  in  the  earthworm 
crawling,2'3  or  the  insect  in  flight,  or  the  fish  swimming,  every 
observer  admits  the  coadjustment  is  essentially  reflex.  One  act 
succeeds  another  without  confusion.  Yet,  tracing  this  sequence 
to  its  external  causes,  we  recognize  that  the  usual  thing  in 
nature  is  not  for  one  exciting  stimulus  to  begin  immediately 
after  another  ceases,  but  for  an  array  of  environmental  agents 
acting  concurrently  on  the  animal  at  any  moment  to  exhibit  cor- 
relative change  in  regard  to  it,  so  that  one  or  other  group  of 
them  becomes  —  generally  by  increase  in  intensity  —  tempo- 
rarily prepotent.  Thus  there  dominates  now  this  group,  now 
that  group,  in  turn.  It  may  happen  that  one  stimulus  ceases 
coincidently  as  another  begins,  but  as  a  rule  the  stimuli  over- 
lap one  another  in  regard  to  time.  Thus  each  reflex  in  the 
unmutilated  animal  breaks  in  upon  a  condition  of  relative 
equilibrium ;  and  this  latter  is  itself  reflex. 

It  was  shown  that  reflex  movements  can  be  grouped  as 
regards  their  mutual  relation  into  those  which  have  allied  rela- 
tion and  mutually  facilitate  and  reinforce,  and  those  which  are 
mutually  antagonistic.     Antagonistic  reflexes  do  not  enter  into 


VI]  SEQUENCE   OF  ALLIED   REFLEXES  183 

simultaneous  combination.  Simultaneous  combination  unites 
"  allied  "  reflexes  only.  But  into  reflex  combinations  of  suc- 
cessive kind,  reflexes  both  allied  and  antagonistic  enter  as 
components. 

If  the  scratch-reflex  be  excited  from  a  spot  in  its  receptive 
skin-field  and  then  while  the  reflex  is  in  progress  another 
scratch-reflex  is  excited  from  a  receptive  point  not  far  removed 
from  the  first  one,  the  scratch-reflex  under  the  double  excitation 
may  difl'er  very  little  in  appearance  from  that  first  excited,  and 
on  the  first  stimulation  being  discontinued  the  reflex  persists, 
maintained  by  the  second  stimulation,  and  hardly  or  not  per- 
ceptibly altered  in  character  from  its  outset.  In  this  case  the 
second  reflex  which  succeeded  the  first  resembles  the  first ;  that 
is,  it  is  to  outward  appearance  and  for  practical  purposes  merely 
a  prolongation  of  it. 

But  if  the  point  of  application  of  the  second  stimulus 
be,  although  still  in  the  receptive  field  of  the  scratch-reflex, 
widely  distant  from  that  of  the  first  stimulus,  the  reflex  be- 
comes obviously  modified  when  the  second  stimulus  is  thrown 
in.  Thus,  if  the  stimulus  be  so  located  in  the  receptive  field 
that  the  first  excites  the  low  form  of  the  reflex  and  the  second 
the  high  form,  the  reflex,  though  initiated  in  the  low  form, 
assumes  the  high  form  when  the  second  stimulus  —  if  that  is 
of  appropriate  strength  —  is  thrown  in.  Or,  conversely,  it  as- 
sumes the  low  form  if  the  second  stimulus  be  appropriately 
located  for  producing  that  form. 

And  between  the  component  scratch-reflexes  there  are  grades 
of  likeness  corresponding  with  degrees  of  proximity  of  the  points 
of  application  of  the  stimuli  in  the  receptive  field.  When,  there- 
fore, a  reflex  occurs  in  immediate  sequence  to  a  reflex  to  which 
it  is  allied,  it  smoothly  maintains  the  reaction  that  is  already  in 
progress,  and  if  its  own  character  differs  in  some  respect  from 
the  foregoing  reflex,  impresses  that  character  on  the  reflex 
without,  however,  any  hitch  or  hindrance  of  the  reflex. 

For  a  reflex  to  be  immediately  succeeded  by  a  reflex  of  allied 
relation  to  it  is  of  common  occurrence.  Any  stimulus  that  moves 
over  a  receptive  field  is  likely  to  excite  such  a  sequence.     A 


i84  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

morsel  of  food  moving  over  the  surface  of  the  tongue,  a  stimulus 
moving  in  the  field  of  vision,  an  object  moving  along  the  skin, 
and,  as  an  instance  of  the  last,  a  parasite  travelling  across  the 
receptive  field  of  the  scratch-reflex.^^ 

In  such  successive  combinations  the  reflexes  are,  in  the 
scratch-reflex  at  least,  linked  together  by  more  than  the  mere 
external  circumstance  of  the  incidence  of  the  stimulus.  In  such 
a  sequence  the  threshold  of  each  succeeding  reflex  is  lowered  by  the 
excitation  just  preceeding  its  own. 

A  subliminal  stimulus  applied  at  a  point  A  will  render  a 
subliminal  stimulus  applied  at  a  point  B  near  A  supraliminal  if 
the  second  stimulus  follow  within  a  short  time,  e.  g.  500  <r.  The 
space  of  receptive  surface  across  which  this  can  be  demonstrated 
in  the  scratch-reflex  amounts  to  5-6  centimeters.  It  is  best 
worked  out  by  unipolar  application  of  the  induced  current 
through  a  stigmatic  electrode  —  fine  gilt  entomological  pin. 
In  that  way  numerical  values  can  be  assigned  to  the  results. 
But  the  phenomenon  is  characteristically  and  simply  illustrated  ^^ 
by  the  difference  between  the  potency  as  a  stimulus  of  the  edge 
of  a  card,  say  six  inches  long,  pressed  simultaneously  its  whole 
length  against  the  receptive  skin-field,  say  for  5  seconds,  and 
on  the  other  hand  lightly  drawing  one  corner  of  the  card  along 
the  same  line  in  the  skin-field  also  for  5  seconds.  The  former 
application  simply  evokes  a  reflex  of  a  few  beats,  which  then 
dies  out  The  latter  evokes  a  vigorous  reflex  that  continues  and 
outlasts  the  application  of  the  stimulus.  A  successive  line  is 
more  effective  as  stimulus  than  a  simultaneous  line  of  equal 
length  and  duration.  Again,  if  a  light  disc  three  centimeters  in 
diameter  and  a  fraction  of  a  millimeter  thick  be  freely  pivoted 
in  bearings  at  the  end  of  a  handle,  so  that  it  turns  when  pushed 
by  its  handle  over  the  skin  surface,  such  a  wheel  may  not,  when 
pushed  against  a  spot  of  the  receptive  surface,  excite  the  reflex, 
but  it  excites  it  when  it  is  rolled  along  it.  The  same  thing  is 
seen  with  a  spur  wheel.  Even  when  the  points  are  two  centi- 
meters apart,  as  the  spur  wheel  is  rolled  over  the  surface  succes- 
sive summation  occurs,  and  the  reflex  is  evoked  as  the  progress 
of  the  wheel  proceeds.     If  a  parasite  in  its  travel  produces  ex- 


VI]  IMMEDIATE   INDUCTION  185 

citation  which  is  but  close  below  the  threshold,  its  progress  is 
likely  to  so  develop  the  excitability  of  the  surface  whither  it 
passes  that  the  scalptor-reflex  will  be  evoked.  In  the  skin 
and  the  parasite  respectively  we  have,  no  doubt,  two  compet- 
ing adaptations  at  work.  It  is  perhaps  to  avoid  the  conse- 
quences of  the  spatial  spread  of  the  "  bahnung "  that  the  hop 
of  the  flea  has  been  developed. 

This  bahnungy  which  spreads  around  a  stimulated  point,  must 
be  the  same  phenomenon  which  finds  still  more  marked  ex- 
pression in  the  summation  of  stimuli  individually  subliminal 
applied  successively  at  one  and  the  same  point.  That  it  in- 
fluences other  points  in  the  neighbourhood  as  well  as  its  own 
seat  of  application,  is  another  item  of  evidence  of  the  central 
conjunction  of  the  neighbouring  reflex-arcs  of  any  one  type-reflex. 
It  is  an  influence  which,  with  barely  supraliminal  stimuli,  is  short 
lived ;  but  it  is  one  of  the  factors  in  the  card-experiment  just 
mentioned.  This  spread  of  influence  to  adjacent  points  is  im- 
portant because,  though  short  lived,  it  can  contribute  efl"ectively 
to  maintain  one  and  the  same  reflex.  It  favours  the  occurrence 
of  sequences  of  closely  allied  reflexes.  It  is  convenient  to  have 
a  term  for  such  a  species  of  "  bahnung^'  and  "  immediate  induc- 
tion "  seems  the  most  suitable  here. 

Phenomena  akin  to  these  are  met  with  in  the  physiology 
of  vision.  A  moving  point  in  the  peripheral  field  is  more 
visible  than  a  line  of  similar  length,  direction,  and  duration. 
Again,  a  row  of  dots  individually  below  the  minimum  visibile  and 
too  far  apart  for  their  retinal  images  to  overlap  by  diffusion, 
becomes  visible.  Probably  this  same  process  is  contributory 
toward  the  seeing  of  lines  the  diameter  of  which  is  narrower 
than  the  diameter  of  the  circular  minimum  visibile.  I  have  found 
osmic-stained  nerve-fibres  of  4  ft  diameter  visible  to  the  naked 
eye  both  for  myself  and  other  workers  in  the  laboratory.  Re- 
inforcement by  positive  induction  appears  to  be  at  work  in  these 
visual  effects  just  as  in  the  scratch-reflex. 

In  the  sequence  of  reflexes  the  supervening  reflex  may  differ 
imperceptibly  or  slightly,  though  distinctly,  from  the  precedent, 
or  may  be  in  part  quite  different  from  that 


1 86 


REFLEX   SEQUENCE 


[Lect. 


Figure  50.  —  The  scratch-reflex  initiated  from  one  receptive  spot  A  in  the  form  of  a  "  low  " 
reflex  and  then  concurrently  from  a  receptive  spot  B,  lying  more  dorsal  than  A,  in  the 
form  of  a  "high"  reflex,  the  tonic  flexion  at  hip  supporting  the  clonic  being  greater. 
Time  above  in  .2  seconds.  Below,  the  upper  signal  marks  the  period  of  excitation  of 
spot  A,  the  lower  signal  that  of  spot  B.  The  line  between  the  two  signals  indicates  the 
rhythm  of  the  induction  shocks  delivered  as  stimuli. 


VI]    TRANSITION    FROM    REFLEX  TO   REFLEX     187 


Figure  51.  —  Effect  of  overlapping  in  time  of  the  stimuli  for  the  flexion- reflex  and  the 
scratch-reflex.  The  top  signal  shows  the  stimulus  for  the  scratch -reflex,  the  lower 
signal  that  for  the  flexion-reflex.  Time  in  seconds  below.  The  scratch-reflex  seems 
to  displace  the  flexion-reflex ;  had  it  fused  with  it  the  combined  lift  of  the  lever  would 
have  been  much  higher  than  it  is  (spinal  dog).  Compare  the  scratch-reflex  above  in  the 
figure  to  left. 

If  the  reflexes  are  closely  similar,  it  is  difficult  to  say  at  what 
moment  the  transition  under  overlapping  stimuli  occurs,  since 
the  initial  reflex  on  discontinuance  of  the  first  stimulation  is 


1 88  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

maintained  unaltered  by  the  second.  But  if  the  reflexes  are 
recognizably  dissimilar,  e.  g.  a  low  scratch-reflex  and  a  high 
scratch-reflex,  the  moment  of  transition  is  obvious,  for  the 
reflex  then  takes  the  form  of  the  response  which  the  second 
stimulation  would  excite,  and,  on  discontinuing  the  first  stim- 
ulation, is  continued  in  that  form  (Fig.  50).  In  my  experience 
the  transition  does  not,  in  the  case  of  this  reflex  at  least,  in- 
clude a  period  of  summation  of  the  two  reflexes  in  the  sense 
that  the  reflex  under  the  two  stimulations  A  and  B  consists  of 
the  response  to  stimulation  A  summed  with  that  to  stimula- 
tion B.  So  also  in  the  transition  from  one  reflex  to  another 
of  even  greater  dissimilarity.  ^Hiere  is  the  same  absence  of 
any  period  of  fusion  of  the  two  reflexes.  Such  fusion  might 
be  appropriately  termed  confusion.  The  rule  seems  that  such 
confusion  is  avoided  in  the  transition.  In  the  hind  limb  a 
scratch-reflex  of  the  high  form  presents  a  certain  degree  of 
resemblance  to  a  simple  flexion-reflex,  inasmuch  as  there  can 
be  distinguished  in  the  former  a  marked  tonic  flexion  on 
which  the  clonic  is,  so  to  say,  superposed.  When  a  scratch- 
reflex  of  this  foMn  supervenes  on  the  flexion-reflex  the  result 
is  commonly  that  which  is  seen  in  Fig.  51.  The  transition 
occurs  without  confusion;  even  in  regard  to  the  tonic  con- 
traction, an  element  possibly  common  to  the  two  reflexes, 
there  is  in  the  transition  no  period  at  which  the  tonic  con- 
traction of  the  flexion-reflex  has  added  to  it  that  of  the 
scratch-reflex.  In  the  instance  figured  the  amplitude  of  the 
tonic  contraction  of  the  scratch-reflex  was  about  equal  to  that 
of  the  flexion-reflex;  if  these  summed,  therefore,  their  joint  am- 
plitude would  be  much  greater  than  that  of  either  reflex  singly. 
But  the  record  shows  a  smooth  continuity  of  flexion-reflex  with 
scratch-reflex  in  which  there  is  no  stage  of  summated  amplitude 
of  the  two  contractions.  This  is  what  I  mean  by  saying  that 
the  transition  from  one  reflex  to  another  takes  place  without 
confusion. 

Not  that  the  onset  of  one  reflex  is  uninfluenced  by  the  exist- 
ence of  the  other  —  its  interval  of  latency  is,  for  instance,  liable 
to  be  greatly  influenced.     In  the  example  furnishing  Fig.  $1, 


VI] 


AVOIDANCE  OF   CONFUSION 


189 


Figure  52.  —  The  displacement  of  the  stepping-reflex  by  the  scratch-reflex  during  the  con- 
tinuance of  the  stimulation  appropriate  for  the  former.  The  upper  signal  indicates  the 
stimulus  for  the  scratch-reflex,  namely,  75  break  shocks  delivered  (unipolar  faradization) 
at  rate  of  30  per  second.  The  lower  signal  gives  the  stimulus  for  the  stepping-reflex  — 
a  crossed  reflex,  the  stimulus  (unipolar  faradization)  being  delivered  to  the  opposite  foot. 
The  scratch-reflex,  after  a  considerable  latency,  displaces  the  stepping-reflex.  The  crossed 
stepping-reflex  reappears  only  in  modified  and  imperfect  form,  though  its  stimulus  is 
continued  unaltered  for  some  seven  seconds  after  the  end  of  the  stimulus  for  the  scratch- 
reflex.    Time  marked  below  in  seconds. 

the  scratch-reflex,  though  its  intensity  is  not  increased,  shows 
a  hesitancy  about  the  opening  of  its  clonus  not  present  in  the 
reflex  when  elicited  singly.  Again,  in  Fig.  52,  which  shows 
transition  from  the  crossed  stepping-reflex  to  the  scratch-reflex, 
though  no  period  of  confusion  occurs  in  the  transition  from  the 
former  reflex  to  the  latter,  there  is  yet,  on  cessation  of  the  latter, 


I90  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

evidence  of  modification  of  the  former.  The  crossed  stepping- 
reflex  returns,  but  considerably  modified  and  after  a  longer 
latency  than  before.  The  amount  of  modification  is  greater 
than  would  in  my  experience  be  ascribable  with  probability  to 
the  effect  of  mere  fatigue  for  the  period  during  which  the  stim- 
ulus was  at  work,  although  the  movement  itself  was  in  abeyance; 
the  rhythm  is  present  but  weakened. 

If  it  is  advantageous  for  the  transition  from  one  reflex  to 
another  of  like  type  to  occur  without  a  period  of  confusion,  it  is 
still  more  advantageous  that  it  should  be  so  in  the  case  of  tran- 
sitions from  one  reflex  to  another  of  converse  type.  Confusion 
in  the  literal  sense  above  would  in  that  case  involve  not  merely 
inaccuracy  at  the  outset  of  each  new  reflex,  but  would  mean 
mutual  destruction  of  the  two  reflex  effects ;  an  interval  of  impo- 
tent mutual  self-hindrance  would  disadvantageously  intervene 
between  successive  motor  acts  of  opposite  direction.  In  the 
transition  from  one  reflex  to  another  of  antagonistic  kind  the 
avoidance  of  confusion  of  the  two  reflexes  emphasizes  at  the 
same  time  the  impossibility  of  co-ordinating  them  in  a  simul- 
taneous combination. 

Though  the  stimulus  exciting  the  reflex  that  is  displaced 
continues  while  the  new  reflex  is  introduced,  the  displacement 
of  the  former  reflex  occurs  without  confusion. 

Taking  the  flexion-reflex  and  the  scratch-reflex,  the  one  may 
temporarily  interrupt  the  other  in  mid  career  (Figs.  43,  53)  or 
may  cut  it  short  or  may  defer  its  onset;  in  all  these  cases  it 
does  so  without  a  phase  of  confusion  in  the  transition,  although 
the  stimuli  belonging  to  both  reflexes  continue  in  contemporary 
operation  (Figs.  43,  51,  52,  etc.)  all  the  time.  And  the  same 
holds  between  other  antagonistic  reflexes,  e.  g.  flexion-reflex 
and  crossed  extension-reflex  (Figs.  30,  32,  33),  extension-reflex 
and  scratch-reflex  (Figs.  42,  54),  etc. 

And  the  direction  of  the  interference  is  reversible.  The 
flexion-reflex  may  be  made  to  interrupt  the  scratch-reflex  (Fig. 
43)  or  the  scratch-reflex  to  interrupt  the  flexion-reflex  (Fig.  51). 
The  scratch-reflex  may  be  made  to  interrupt  the  crossed  step- 
ping-reflex  (Fig.  52)  or  the  crossed  stepping-reflex  to  interrupt 


INHIBITION 


191 


Figure  53.  —  A.  Scratch-reflex  interrupted  by  a  brief  flexion-reflex.  The  time  of  applica- 
tion of  the  stimulus  evoking  scratch-reflex  is  shown  by  the  lowest  signal  line;  that  of  the 
stimulus  of  the  flexion-reflex  in  the  signal  line  immediately  above  the  other.  Time 
marked  in  fifths  of  seconds  at  top  of  the  record.  The  scratch-reflex  returns  with  in- 
creased intensity  after  the  interruption. 

B.  Similar  to  A,  but  the  scratch-reflex  is  interrupted  later  and  returns  more  slowly  and 
with  marked  irregularity  in  its  beat. 

the  scratch-reflex  (Fig.   55);  and  similarly  with  other  pairs  of 
antagonistic  reflexes. 

Inhibition.  The  process  in  virtue  of  which  this  transition 
from  one  antagonistic  reflex  to  another  occurs  is  obviously  one 
of  active  intervention.  In  many  cases  the  form  which  the  in- 
tervention takes  is  inhibition.  For  instance,  where  a  crossed 
extension-reflex  is  interrupted  by  a  brief  flexion-reflex,  two 
transitions  occur,  the  first  at  a  moment  a  from  the  E-reflex  to 
the  F-reflex,  the  second  at  a  moment  /3  from  the  F-reflex  back 
to  the  E-reflex  (Fig.  33,  p.  loi).  Although  stimuli  for  the  two 
reflexes  are  both  in  operation  continuously  from  a  to  y8,  there 
is  a  clean  transition  from  one  reflex  to  the  other.  The  tracing 
is  taken  from  the  extensor  muscle  of  the  knee.  The  flexion- 
reflex  expresses  itself  by  inhibition  of  the  reflex  contraction  in 
progress  in  that  muscle  at  that  time  under  the  combined  in- 
fluence of  the  crossed  extension-reflex  and  of  the  tonic  extensor 


192  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 


Figure  54.  The  scratch-reflex  cut  short  by  excitation  of  the  skin  of  a  digit  of  the  opposite 
hind  foot.  Below,  the  upper  signal  marks  the  period  of  application  of  the  stimulus  to 
the  opposite  hind  foot,  the  lower  signal  marks  the  period  of  application  of  the  stimulus 
exciting  the  scratch-reflex.    Time  above  in  .2  seconds. 

rigidity  due  to  the  animal  being  decerebrate.  It  causes  the 
contraction  due  to  these  conjoined  reflexes  to  cease.  In  such 
a  case  the  transition  from  the  extension-reflex  to  the  flexion- 
reflex  evidently  occurs  by  inhibition.  So  also  in  the  transition 
from  the  flexion-reflex  to  the  extension-reflex  when  the  ham- 
string muscles  are  examined  (Fig.  32,  p.  98). 

We  do  not  yet  understand  the  intimate  nature  of  inhibition. 
In  the  cases  before  us  now,  its  seat  is  certainly  central,  and  in  all 
probability  is,  as  argued  above,  situated  at  points  of  synapsis. 
I  have  urged  that  a  prominent  physiological  feature  of  the 
synapse  is  a  synaptic  membrane.  It  seems  therefore  to  me 
that  inhibition  in  such  cases  as  those  before  us  is  probably 
referable  to  a  change  in  the  condition  of  the  synaptic  membrane 
causing  a  block  in  conduction.  But  what  the  intimate  nature 
of  the  inhibitory  change  may  be  we  do  not  know. 

The  views  of  some  of  those  who  have  authoritatively  treated 


VI]  INHIBITION  193 

of  the  nature  of  inhibition  may  be  cited  here,  both  for  their  in- 
trinsic interest  and  the  suggestion  of  lines  of  investigation.  One 
view  has  been  that,  as  the  process  of  conduction  along  nerve- 
fibres  is  an  undulatory  one  in  the  sense  that  the  nerve-impulse 
travels  as  a  disturbance  with  wave-like  configuration  of  inten- 
sity, inhibition  is  due  to  a  mutual  suppression  of  two  wave-like 
disturbances  impinging  on  the  same  point  of  the  conductor  but 
in  opposite  phases  of  disturbance. 

In  those  cases  where  stimulation  through  one  nerve  inhibits 
the  action  of  a  tissue  acting  under  another  nerve  we  can  imagine 
a  process  which  leaves  the  tissue  unaffected  but  simply  inter- 
feres with  another  stimulus,  as  in  the  physical  interference  of 
vibrations.  Rosenthal's  ^  resistance  theory  of  the  action  of  the 
afferent  vagus-fibres  upon  the  respiratory  centre  is  a  supposition 
of  this  kind.  It  recognizes  during  the  inhibition  no  change  of 
total  output  of  energy  in  the  particular  function  inhibited. 
The  alteration  of  a  hypothetical  resistance  only  distributes  the 
discharge  of  the  nerve-centre  over  an  altered  time-rhythm, — 
smaller  and  more  frequent  discharges  representing  the  same  lib- 
eration of  energy  as  larger  and  less  frequent.  Such  a  view  of  the 
nature  of  inhibition  is  that  which  Gaskell  ^^  termed  the  "  neutra/" 
one ;  according  to  it  the  inhibition  leaves  the  tissue  in  the  same 
ultimate  condition  as  that  in  which  it  found  it,  neither  exhausted 
nor  surcharged.  "  As  far  as  the  central  nervous  system  is  con- 
cerned, there  exists  a  strong  general  tendency  to  look  upon 
the  inhibitory  processes  occurring  there  as  *  neutraV  in  their 
character. "  ^^ 

This  view,  originally  put  forward  by  v.  Cyon,^  has  been 
abandoned  by  him,  although  it  has  since  been  supported  by 
Lauder  Brunton  ^^'  and  others.  It  was  expressly  dissented  from 
by  Wundt.^^  And  the  grounds  of  Wundt's  objection  are  valid, 
the  kernel  of  them  being  that  though  in  a  certain  sense  of  the 
word  the  nerve-impulse  can  be  described  as  wave-like  it  is  not 
an  undulatory  disturbance  at  all  in  the  sense  in  which  those 
reactions  are  which  show  physical  interference.  It  is  therefore 
to  physical  interference  that  in  this  view  of  inhibition  the 
analogy  is  drawn,  but  the  similarity  between  the  process  of 

13 


I 


194  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 


Figure  55.  —  The  scratch-reflex  interrupted  by  the  crossed  stepping-reflex.  The  upper 
signal  of  the  two  below  the  myograph  trace  shows  the  period  of  application  of  the  stimu- 
lation (unipolar  faradization  of  shoulder  skin)  evoking  the  scratch-reflex.  During  the  ; 
continuance  of  this  stimulation  a  fairly  strong  stimulation  of  the  crossed  foot  was  applied, 
as  marked  by  the  lower  signal  at  bottom  of  the  record.  The  inhibition  outlasts  the 
application  of  this  second  stimulus  by  some  3  seconds ;  the  scratch-reflex  then  returns, 
and  only  ceases  on  cessation  of  its  own  stimulus.  Time  marked  in  fifths  of  seconds  • 
above. 

nerve-conduction  and  conduction  of  light  and  electrical  waves  \ 

or  sound,  etc.,  is  not  real  enough  to  strictly  justify  such  analogy.  ; 

Moreover,  as  we  shall  see  presently,  central  inhibition  is  not  a  : 

neutral  process,  for,  at  least  in  many  cases,  it  leaves  the  reflex  ■ 

centre  surcharged  for  subsequent  response  {v.  infra^  pp.  205--213,  ; 

"successive  induction").  \ 

The  most  striking  thing  that  we  know  of  inhibition  is  that  it  i 

is  a  phenomenon  in  which  an  agent  such  as  in  other  cases  excites  \ 

or  increases  an  action  going  on  in  this  case  stops  or  diminishes  ^ 

an  action  going  on.     Now,  the  activity  of  a  tissue  can  be  low-  ■ 

ered  or  abolished  by  production  in  it  of  deleterious  changes  \ 

such  as  exhaustion  or,  in  the  highest  degree,  death.     But  there  \ 

is  no  evidence  that  inhibition  of  a  tissue  is  ever  accompanied  by  \ 

the  slightest  damage  to  the  tissue ;  on  the  contrary,  it  seems  to  \ 

predispose  the  tissue  to  a  greater  functional  activity  thereafter.  ' 


VI]  INHIBITION  195 

We  can  imagine  that  a  material  continuously  produced  by 
a  tissue,  and  yielding  on  decomposition  the  particular  activity 
which  is  inhibited,  may  by  an  inhibition  be  checked  in  its 
decomposition,  and  accumulate,  so  that  at  the  end  of  the  period 
of  inhibition  the  tissue  contains  more  of  the  particular  decom- 
posable material  than  before.  This  molecular  rearrangement 
would  diminish  activity  for  the  time  being,  but  lead  to  increase 
of  activity  afterwards.  There  would  ensue  a  rebound  effect. 
This  is,  as  is  well  known  since  Gaskell's  ^*  researches,  what 
actually  happens  in  the  pure  vagus  action  on  the  heart.  A 
similar  rebound-effect  is  perfectly  obvious  in  many  instances 
of  inhibition  in  the  central  nervous  system.^^  It  constitutes  a 
point  of  resemblance  between  central  and  peripheral  inhibition. 

Such  an  explanation  as  this  second  one  may  take  the  chem- 
ical structure  of  the  living  material,  the  bioplasnty  of  the  cell  as 
the  field  of  operation  for  the  decomposition  and  the  synthetic 
process  that  it  pictures.  The  living  cell  is  constantly  liberating 
energy  in  its  function,  and  rebuilding  its  complex  structure 
from  nutrient  material.  Its  life  is  therefore  an  equilibrium  of 
balanced  katabolism  and  anabolism ;  at  any  given  moment  the 
one  process  or  the  other  may  predominate  in  the  cell.  At  a 
moment  when  the  cell  is  vigorously  discharging  some  function 
which  involves  conversion  of  internal  energy  into  external  energy 
it  is  in  a  katabolic  phase.  In  subsequent  relative  repose  from 
the  discharge  of  that  function  the  replenishment  of  its  store  of 
potential  energy  by  assimilation  may  predominate  and  the  cell 
be  in  an  anabolic  phase.  Katabolism  and  activity  of  external 
function,  anabolism  and  rest  from  external  function,  come  on 
this  view  to  be  almost  synonymous  terms.  But  Hering,  Gaskell, 
Verworn,  and  others  have  taught  us  to  attach  important  external 
functions  to  the  assimilatory  (anabolic)  phase.  The  first  men- 
tioned has  dealt  with  the  visual  sensations  in  assimilation-dis- 
similation pairs.  Black-white  sensation  is  thus  traced  to  a  pair 
of  reactions  affecting  the  trophism  of  the  cell  in  exactly  opposite 
directions.  Gaskell  relates  vagus  inhibition  of  the  heart  to  the 
throwing  of  the  cardiac  muscle-cell  into  a  phase  predominantly 
anabolic.     Verworn  in  his  "  Biogen-hypothese  "  has  developed 


196  RFFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

this  trophic  theory  with  further  elaboration  still.  These  views 
all  take  the  actual  nutritive  activity  of  the  cell  as  the  direct  and 
immediate  field  in  which  inhibition  has  its  seat.  The  tendency 
to  rebound  after-effect  in  the  opposite  direction  is  in  this  view 
a  natural  trophic  result.  Hence  Gaskell  expressively  speaks  of 
the  vagus  as  the  "trophic  nerve  of  the  heart."  And  Hering*^ 
(1872)^^2  formulated  his  experience  somewhat  as  follows: 
The  action  of  a  stimulus  affects  the  cell's  autonomic  trophic 
equilibrium ;  it  may  increase  or  lessen  either  the  cell's  asssimi- 
lation  or  the  cell's  dissimilation;  in  whichever  of  these  ways  the 
stimulus  acts  the  excitation  of  the  cell,  owing  to  a  self-regulation 
proper  to  it,  dwindles  for  that  stimulus  and  for  all  stimuli  produc- 
ing a  similar  change,  while  the  excitability  increases  for  stimuli 
that  produce  an  opposite  effect.  On  all  these  views  inhibition 
is  in  its  intimate  and  essential  nature  a  trophic  process. 

An  hypothesis  not  built  immediately  on  views  of  the  nutri- 
tive processes  of  the  cell  has  recently  been  put  forward  by  Mac- 
donald.®^  Dealing  with  nerve  and  muscle  fibres,  he  does  not 
take  the  purely  chemical  structure  of  the  living  framework  of 
the  cell  as  the  field  of  operation  for  either  excitation  or  inhibi- 
tion. The  explanation  he  offers  of  these  two  latter  processes 
is  as  follows. 

From  study  of  the  part  played  by  inorganic  salts  in  the 
function  of  nerve,  he  sees  in  the  attachment  of  these  salts  to 
the  proteids  present,  and  in  their  partial  detachment  from  the 
proteids,  normal  occurrences  underlying  the  conditions  of  rest 
and  excitation  respectively.  He  assumes  the  connection  be- 
tween salt  and  proteid  involved  in  this  matter  to  be  of  purely 
physical  nature.  The  axis-cylinder,  for  example,  is  composed 
of  a  colloid  solution  in  which  there  are  present  minute  particles 
of  colloid  proteid.  These  may  increase  in  size  —  indeed  so 
much  as  finally  to  become  visible  —  under  the  influence  of 
factors  determining  a  tendency  towards  coagulation.  Upon  the 
surface  of  these  particles  the  major  portion  of  the  inorganic  salts 
present  is  held  restrained  in  a  condition  of  condensed  solution. 
An  increase  in  the  size  of  the  particles  is  accompanied  by  a 
diminution  in  the  total  surface  separating  the  particles  from  the 


VI]  INHIBITION  197 

solvent  in  which  they  lie.  Such  a  diminution  in  surface  is 
equivalent  to  a  diminution  in  the  forces  restraining  the  motion 
of  the  inorganic  salts.  It  occasions  the  liberation  of  salt  mole- 
cules in  a  state  of  free  motion  into  the  surrounding  aqueous 
solution.  This  release  of  hitherto  restrained  molecules  is  the 
cause  of  alterations  in  osmotic  pressure,  of  new  processes  of 
diffusion,  of  resultant  electrical  phenomena,  and  thus  of  the 
phenomena  of  excitation. 

Macdonald  thus  considers  a  stimulus  as  an  agency  determin- 
ing an  approach  to  the  condition  of  coagulation.  He  regards 
as  the  important  characteristic  of  the  excited  state  the  release 
of  inorganic  salts  resulting  from  this  change.  According  to  him 
inhibition  is  a  condition  in  which  the  inorganic  salts  are  more 
securely  packed  away  upon  the  surface  of  the  "  colloid  particles  " 
than  usual  by  reason  of  diminution  in  the  individual  size  of  the 
particles  and  an  increase  in  the  surface  they  present  to  the  sur- 
rounding solution.  He  supposes  the  communication  of  a  nega- 
tive electrical  charge  to  be  a  stimulus  provoking  a  tendency  to 
coagulation  with  all  the  just-mentioned  dynamic  consequences 
of  the  enlargement  of  the  colloid  particles.  So  he  considers 
that  conversely  the  communication  of  a  positive  charge  produces 
a  change  of  an  exactly  opposite  kind  in  which  inorganic  salts 
hitherto  in  motion  are  brought  to  a  static  condition  of  rest. 

It  does  not  seem  at  once  clear  that  the  condition  of  greater 
subdivision  of  particles  should  also  be  a  condition  of  greater 
stability,  although  it  is  clear  that  such  a  conception  might  ex- 
plain the  greater  store  of  potential  energy  possessed  by  an 
inhibited  tissue.  Macdonald,  in  fact,  does  not,  I  take  it,  assume 
this  to  be  the  case.  To  explain  this  separate  fact  he  appeals  to 
the  evidence  upon  which  the  conception  is  based  and  points  out 
a  distinction  between  the  amount  of  inorganic  salt  involved  in 
changes  above  (inhibition)  and  changes  below  (excitation)  the 
equilibrium  line  of  the  normal  resting  state.  Thus  there  is 
quantitative  evidence  to  show  that  the  amount  of  salt  remaining 
for  withdrawal  from  the  resting  colloid  solution  is  only  a  small 
fraction  of  the  total  amount  of  salt  present ;  the  major  portion 
is  already  withdrawn,  and  is,  so  to  speak,  in  reserve  in  this  large 


198  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

quantity  for  such  changes  of  excitation  as  occur  below  the  base 
line  of  rest.  Changes  from  the  normal  to  the  hypernormal,  and 
from  the  hypernormal  to  the  normal,  cannot  therefore  involve  a 
redistribution  of  salt  comparable  in  quantity  to  those  taking 
place  down  from,  and  up  to,  the  normal.  It  is  in  this  way  con- 
ceivable that  the  application  of  a  stimulus  to  an  inhibited  tissue 
—  although  productive  of  an  effect  akin  to  the  phenomenon  un- 
derlying the  process  of  excitation  —  might  yet  lead  only  to  such 
a  subminimal  change  in  the  amount  of  inorganic  salt  in  motion 
as  to  determine  no  externally  appreciable  manifestation  of  its 
occurrence.  Imagine  a  tissue  which  has  been  placed  in  a  con- 
dition of  inhibition  by  the  communication  of  a  positive  electrical 
charge.  The  application  of  a  negative  charge  to  such  a  tissue 
would  produce  no  visible  effect,  although  productive  of  an  in- 
ternal change.  The  application  of  a  second  negative  charge 
would  give  rise  to  a  characteristic  excitation,  its  efficiency  deter- 
mined by  "  summation."  Let  us  on  the  other  hand  suppose 
that  the  tissue  has  not  only  been  inhibited,  but  is  maintained  in 
a  continuous  state  of  inhibition  by  the  steady  arrival  of  positive 
charges.  In  this  case  the  application  of  a  succession  of  stimuli 
would  result  in  nothing  more  than  a  series  of  subminimal,  and 
therefore  unnoticed,  changes. 

In  an  excited  tissue,  summarizing  this  conception,  an  unusual 
quantity  of  inorganic  salts  is  in  motion.  Excitation  is  ended  by 
the  reduction  of  this  excessive  motion.  Inhibition  is  the  condi- 
tion in  which  the  possibilities  of  free  motion  are  most  reduced. 

This  view  is  fertile  in  suggestion  for  further  experiment. 
Based  on  examination  of  the  physical  structure  of  nerve  by 
electrical  methods  irreproachably  employed,  and  on  the  revela- 
tion, under  the  microchemical  test  which  we  owe  to  Macallum,^' 
of  potassium  appearing  in  quantity  at  injured  points  of  nerve- 
fibres,  and  explaining  naturally  as  it  does  the  injury-current  of 
nerve  as  similar  in  production  to  the  current  of  a  "  concentra- 
tion "  battery  2^*  the  concentrations  of  which  can  be  known  from 
the  current,  it  merits  very  careful  consideration.  The  features 
and  conditions  of  occurrence  of  inhibition  harmonize  strikingly 
with  what  on  Macdonald's  view  we  should  expect  them  to  be. 


^I]  INTERFERENCE  199 


I 

^m  Interference.  Whatever  the  intimate  nature  of  the  inhibition, 
^Ht  is,  however,  only  one  part  of  the  processes  involved  in  transi- 
^Hon  from  one  antagonistic  reflex  to  another.  In  the  transition 
^Brom  the  crossed  extension-reflex  to  the  flexion-reflex  the  in- 
hibition of  previous  excitation  in  the  extensor  neurone  is  accom- 
panied by  excitation  of  the  previously  inhibited  flexor  neurone. 
And  conversely  in  the  transition  from  flexion-reflex  to  extension- 
reflex.  Transition  from  one  form  of  reciprocal  innervation  to 
another  will  obviously  involve  such  changes  wherever  the  transi- 
tion is  from  one  reflex  of  simultaneous  double-sign  to  an  antago- 
nistic of  simultaneous  double-sign.  There  will  be  inhibition  at 
one  set  of  points  and  excitation  at  another.  The  process  of 
transition,  therefore,  in  many  cases  is  one  half  of  it  inhibitory, 
one  half  excitatory.  It  seems  advisable,  therefore,  to  avoid  em- 
ploying the  term  "inhibition"  for  the  displacement  in  general  of 
one  reflex  by  another.  To  avoid  confusion,  some  expression  of 
broader  scope,  including  excitation  as  well  as  inhibition  proper, 
seems  required.  The  term  "  interference "  already  used  by 
Wundt®^  and  by  A.  Tschermak^^  in  an  almost  similar  way  seems 
to  me  well  suited  for  this  purpose.  In  employing  it  Wundt 
expressly  stipulated  that  it  had  in  this  use  no  reference  to  its 
technical  employment  by  physicists  for  the  mutual  interaction 
of  vibrations,  as  in  light  and  sound.  The  term  "  interference  " 
as  applied  to  reflexes  would  mean  simply  the  interaction  between 
antagonistic  reflexes,  that  is,  reflexes  which  are  incapable  of 
simultaneous  combination.  These  reflexes  are  capable  of  sue- . 
cessive  combination,  and  in  that  process  the  influence  of  one 
reflex  replaces  that  of  another  upon  a  common  path  potentially 
belonging  to  each.  The  replacement  may  take  place  by  inhi- 
bition succeeding  excitation,  or  by  excitation  succeeding  in- 
hibition, or  by  excitation  of  one  kind  succeeding  excitation  of 
another  kind,  as  when  the  steady  tonic  flexion  of  the  flexion- 
reflex  is  succeeded  by  the  rhythmic  clonic  flexion  of  the 
scratch-reflex.  In  all  these  cases  the  process  of  displacement 
of  one  reflex  action  by  the  other  may  be  termed  interference. 
We  thus  get  a  comprehensive  convenient  term  for  embracing 
the  whole  series  of  cases. 


200  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

The  frequency  with  which,  in  co-ordination  of  reflexes  by 
successive  combination,  the  reflex  which  succeeds  to  another  is 
antagonistic  to  this  latter  is  very  great.  Two  important  classes 
of  such  sequence  are  especially  common.  One  is  that  which 
forms  what  are  known  as  ''alternating  reflexes;"  the  other  is 
the  class  of  "  compensatory  reflexes." 

Alternating  reflexes  are  seen  very  clearly  in  cyclic  reversals 
of  direction  of  movement;  thus,  when  extension  succeeds 
flexion  in  the  stepping-reflex.  Here  antagonistic  reflexes  suc- 
ceed one  another  alternately  at  two  final  common  paths.  In 
the  motor  neurones  for  the  knee,  in  the  stepping-reflex,  excita- 
tion and  inhibition  alternately  ensue  in  the  flexor  neurones,  while 
synchronously  with  that,  inhibition  and  excitation  alternately 
ensue  in  the  extensor  neurones. 

The  essence  of  an  alternating  reflex  is  that  excitation  and 
inhibition  ensue  in  succession  at  two  (or  more)  final  common 
paths,  a  sequence  of  antagonistic  reflexes  possessing  them  in 
turn.  In  an  ordinary  rhythmic  reflex  a  periodic  excitation 
(and  a  periodic  refractory  or  inhibitory  state)  is  recurrently 
produced  in  the  reflex-arc  at  rhythmic  intervals.  Every  alter- 
nating reflex,  therefore,  is  a  rhythmic  reflex,  but  not  every 
rhythmic  reflex  is  an  alternating  reflex.  The  movements  of  the 
vertebrate  limb  in  locomotion  give  instances  of  alternating  re- 
flexes ;  probably  the  movement  of  the  tail  of  fishes  in  swimming 
is  a  similar  instance,  but  has  not  yet  been  analyzed  as  to  its  reflex 
composition.  Alternating  reflexes  form  an  excellent  field  for 
examination  of  reciprocal  innervation  of  antagonistic  muscles. 

It  is  particularly  by  the  case  of  "  alternating  reflexes  "  that 
Macdougall  illustrates  his  view  of  the  central  process  in  re- 
ciprocal innervation.  His  scheme  also  offers  an  explanation 
for  the  transition  from  one  antagonistic  reflex  to  another. 
Based  more  immediately  on  the  behaviour  of  visual  images, 
it  is  applied  by  Macdougall  specifically  to  the  case  of  the 
reciprocal  innervation  of  antagonistic  muscles.  "Let  us,"  he 
writes,^®*  "imagine  each  arc  in  a  simple  schematic  form  as  a 
chain  of  three  neurones  afferent  (^j),  central  (^a^,  efferent 
(^3),  and  let  us  call  them  a^,  a^,  and  ^3,  and  ^j,  b^,  and  ^3,  in 


VI]  ALTERNATING   REFLEXES  201 


exTCNsoit 


Figure  56.  —  Explanation  in  text. 

the  two  arcs  respectively"  (Fig.  55).  "When  a  strong  stim- 
ulus is  applied  to  the  afferent  neurone  of  arc  a  it  generates 
neurin  rapidly,  so  that  it  becomes  very  rapidly  charged,  and  the 
resistance  of  synapse  a-^  —  a^  is  lowered  until  a  series  of  dis- 
charges takes  place  from  a^  to  ^g*  ^"^  again  from  a^  to  a^.  The 
problem  is,  then,  to  imagine  such  a  mode  of  connection  between 
arc  a  and  arc  b  as  will  cause  arc  a  during  stimulation  to  drain  off 
from  the  afferent  and  central  neurones  of  b  the  smaller  quantities 
of  neurin  generated  in  them.  Several  forms  of  such  a  connection 
may  be  imagined,  but  I  think  that  probably  it  takes  the  form  of 
a  collateral  fibre  coming  from  neurone  b^,  and  taking  part  with 
the  axone  of  a^  in  forming  a  synapse  with  ^3."  "  Whatever  the 
exact  constitution  of  this  synapse  may  be,  we  may  assume  that, 
when  its  resistance  is  lowered  by  the  stimulation  of  a^  and  con- 
sequent charging  of  a^y  the  collateral  of  ^2>  making  connection 
with  a^,  through  this  synapse,  becomes  the  path  of  least  resist- 
ance for  the  escape  of  neurin  from  b^  and  ^2-  These  neurones 
are  therefore  drained  by  a^,  while  b^  ceases  to  receive  any  neurin 
from  ^2»  2i"d  the  tone  of  the  muscle-group  supplied  by  it  is 
abolished."  "  In  a  similar  way,  if  both  a^  and  b^  be  stimulated, 
but  one  more  strongly  than  the  other,  the  more  strongly  stimu- 
lated arc  will  drain  the  afferent  and  central  neurones  of  the  less 
strongly  stimulated  arc,  because  the  resistance  of  synapses  of 
the  former  will  be  reduced  to  a  lower  level  than  that  of  the 
synapses  of  the  latter." 
This  scheme  fits  a  number  of  facts  of  reciprocal  inhibition. 


202  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

Thus,  in  reciprocal  innervation,  as  the  term  itself  implies,  the 
inhibition  at  one  part  always  appears  as  the  negative  aspect  of 
positive  excitation  at  another.  To  "  alternating  "  reflexes,  which 
are  common  as  spinal  reactions,  Macdougall  applies  the  scheme 
thus :  "  We  must  suppose  the  collateral  connection  of  the  arc  a 
(to  which  we  may  suppose  the  stimulus  to  be  applied)  with  the 
neurone  b^  to  be  but  little  inferior  in  conducting  capacity  to 
its  direct  connection  with  a^.  When,  then,  the  neurone  a  is 
stimulated  continuously,  the  arc  will  first  discharge  into  a^  and 
drain  b-^  and  b^  (J.  e.  inhibit  ^3)  until  after  some  little  time  fatigue 
causes  the  resistance  of  synapse  a^  —  a^  to  become  slightly 
greater  than  that  of  synapse  b,^ — ^3,  when  a^  will  discharge 
with  ^2  wholly  into  ^3,  and  a^  will  be  in  turn  "inhibited."  So 
the  charges  of  the  afferent  neurones  of  both  arcs  will  be  dis- 
charged into  ^3  until  fatigue  causes  rise  of  resistance  of  the 
synapse  on  ^3."  The  high  influence  of  intensity  of  reaction 
in  determining  whether  the  reaction  shall  or  shall  not  replace 
another  reaction  is  expressed  by  this  scheme  very  lucidly.  Also 
the  occurrence  under  strychnine^^  and  tetanus  toxin  of  excita- 
tion at  a  synapse  where  inhibition  is  otherwise  the  rule  seems 
a  contingency  which  the  view  answers  well.  If  these  agents 
(strychnine  presumably  from  the  afferent  side,  tetanus  toxin 
from  the  efferent)  reduce  the  resistance  at  synapse  a^ — ^3,  there 
will,  on  stimulation  of  a-^^  be  conversion  of  the  inhibition  of  b^  in 
excitation,  just  as  in  the  second  phase  of  an  alternating  reflex, 
but  the  resistance  at  synapse  a^  —  a^  not  being  raised,  as  it  is 
in  the  second  phase  of  the  alternating  reflex,  a^  will  also  be 
excited  as  usual,  and  both  antagonistic  muscles  will  contract,  as 
I  have  shown  they  do  in  fact  in  strychnine  reflexes  and  as  they 
do  in  the  convulsions  of  strychnine  poisoning.  The  scheme 
makes  it  clear,  too,  that  this  double  discharge,  or  leakage,  will 
prove  rapidly  exhausting  on  the  central  arcs,  and  indeed  the 
phasic  character  of  the  attacks  in  strychnine  poisoning  does  seem 
due  to  rapid  exhaustion  following  each  convulsive  discharge. 

Again,  in  working  with  tetanus  toxin,  I  have,^^  in  the 
gradual  progress  of  the  disease,  several  times  found  the  afferent 
nerves  produce  a  slight  reflex  inhibition  of  the  extensor  of  the 


[]  COMPENSATING   REFLEXES  203 

:nee,  if  the  initial  posture  at  the  knee  were  at  the  time  exten- 
jion  (/*.  e.y  the  extensor  arc  in  high  activity),  and  yet  produce 
listinct  excitation  of  the  extensor  if  the  initial  posture  at  the 
ime  were  flexion  (/.  e.y  the  extensor  arc  in  less  activity).  These 
jffects  of  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  the  view  of  Macdougall 
jeems  well  fitted  to  meet,  though  they  were  not  known  at  the 
ime  the  view  was  formulated.  The  view  seems  also  applicable 
some  of  the  results  obtained  in  the  interesting  experiments 
)y  V.  Uexkiill  on  Invertebrata. 

One  difficulty  however  seems  to  me  presented  by  Macdougall's 

lew,  and  it  is  that  the  diversion  of  the  influence  of  a^  through 

►2  away  from  a^  which  constitutes  inhibition  does  not  suggest 

reason  for  the  superactivity  (successive  induction)  in  a  conse- 

[uent  on  the  inhibition.    And  a  more  serious  difficulty,  attaches, 

in  my  thinking,  to  the  view,  —  at  least,  as  it  at  present  stands  — 

in  that  it  seems  to  sever  this  central  nervous  inhibition  —  of 

which  I  regard  reciprocal  innervation  of  antagonistic  muscles  as 

but  one  widely  spread  case  —  from  other  forms  of  inhibition 

met  peripherally  in  the  heart,  blood-vessels,  and  viscera,  rather 

than  to  connect  it  with  them.     It  appears  to  me  unlikely  that  in 

their  essential  nature  all  forms  of  inhibition  can  be  anything  but 

one  and  the  same  process. 

Another  important  class  of  sequence  of  antagonistic  reflexes 
is  that  which  gives  the  "  compensatory  reflex."  A  compensa- 
tory reflex  occurs  where  the  reflex  is  a  return  to  a  state  of  reflex 
equilibrium  which  had  been  disturbed  by  an  intercurrent  reflex 
to  which  the  compensatory  reflex  is  the  diametrical  antagonist 
Some  compensatory  reflexes  are  excited  by  passive  movements, 
others  by  active  movements.  It  is  the  latter  which  come  under 
consideration  here.  Many  reflex  movements  are  intercurrent 
reactions,  breaking  in  on  a  condition  of  neural  equilibrium  itself 
reflex.  Take  the  case  of  the  flexion-reflex  of  the  leg  (spinal  dog) 
induced  by  a  brief  stimulus  during  "  decerebrate  rigidity,"  where 
the  animal  may  be  regarded  as  a  bulbo-spinal  machine.  Sup- 
pose the  animal  suspended  with  spine  horizontal  and  limbs 
pendent.     The  limbs  are  then  in  slight  active  extension,  much 


204  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

as  if  the  animal  were  standing.  This  slight  extension  is  active, 
for  it  is  reflex,  and  the  peripheral  source  of  the  maintained  re- 
flex pose  is  traceable  to  arcs  arising  in  the  extensor  muscles,  in 
alliance  probably  with  some  from  the  otic  labyrinth.  The  crea- 
ture breathes  quietly  and  regularly.  Its  skeletal  musculature 
otherwise  exhibits  no  movements,  although  its  reflex  activity  is 
considerable  and  in  progress  all  the  time,  as  shown  by  the  steady 
reflex  extension  of  the  limbs.  If,  then,  the  foot  be  excited  by 
a  brief  stimulus  and  thus  a  flexion-reflex  induced  in  the  limb, 
the  limb  is  drawn  up  at  hip,  knee,  and  ankle.  The  movement 
is  brief;  after  being  drawn  up,  the  limb  returns  to  its  previous 
pendent  posture.  It  is  easy  in  many  instances  to  perceive  that 
the  pose  of  extension,  as  resumed,  is  more  marked  than  it  was 
prior  to  the  intercurrent  reflex  or  flexion.  It  is  also  equally 
easy  to  perceive  that  in  the  replacement  of  the  limb  the  exten- 
sion is  not  a  mere  passive  drop  under  gravity,  but  is  a  rever- 
sion to  the  previous  posture  by  an  active  movement  In  such 
a  case  the  reflex  effect  of  the  intercurrent  stimulus  seems  to 
cease  with  the  cessation  of  the  intercurrent  reflex  (flexion) 
which  the  stimulus  immediately  provoked.  But  closer  examina- 
tion shows  that  this  is  not  really  the  case.  There  is  an  active 
reflex  return  to  the  pre-existing  pose.  Thus,  the  disturbing 
stimulus  brought  about  not  only  the  flexion-reflex,  but,  second- 
arily to  that,  a  reflex  antagonistic  to  that.  This  latter  antago- 
nistic (extension)  reflex  is  "  allied  "  to  that  which  originally  held 
the  field.  When  the  flexion-reflex  disturbed  the  neural  equi- 
librium it  dispossessed  the  opposed  reflex  of  extension  from 
certain  final  paths  common  to  that  and  itself.  In  other  words, 
its  own  reaction  induces  an  after-coming  reflex  antagonistic  to 
itself,  and  this  brings  resumption  of  the  original  reflex  attitude 
that,  under  the  condition  (gravity,  etc.)  obtaining  at  the  time, 
satisfies  neural  equilibrium.  The  whole  intercurrent  reflex  dis- 
turbance is  really  ended  by  a  "compensatory  reflex."  The 
compensatory  reflex  in  this  case  seems  traceable  primarily  to 
proprio-ceptive  (muscular)  aflerents  fi-om  the  muscles,  joints, 
etc.,  of  the  limb.  But  compensatory  reflexes  are  particularly 
evident   in   reflex   reactions   started   by  labyrinthine   affe rents 


VI]    FACTORS   DETERMINING   THE   SEQUENCE     205 

(Ewald,  Lee,  Loeb,  Lyon,  Muskens,  Nagel,  and  others),  and  in 
the  decerebrate  animal  this  compensatory  reflex  of  the  limb 
is  presumably  due  to  muscular  afferents  of  the  limbs  and  to 
labyrinthine  afferents  acting  in  reflex  alliance. 

Among  the  afferent  nerves  of  importance  to  this  compensat- 
ing reflex  there  seems  particularly  that  of  the  vasto-crureus  muscle 
itself,  the  extensor  of  the  knee.  Electrical  stimulation  of  the 
central  end  of  that  nerve  excites  contraction  of  the  flexors  of 
hip  and  knee  and  inhibits  the  vasto-crureus  itself.  It  therefore 
reinforces  the  flexion-reflex,  but  its  stimulation  is,  on  being  dis- 
continued, immediately  succeeded  by  contraction  of  the  extensorf  i 
muscles.  This  rebound  (successive  induction)  is  particularly''- 
marked  in  the  vasto-crureus  itself.  These  instances  exemplify, 
further  what  was  said  as  to  the  close  connection  of  secondary, 
not  immediate,  kind  between  reflexes  initiated  by  receptors  of 
the  extero-ceptive  {e.g.  skin)  surface  and  reflexes  initiated  by 
receptors  of  the  deep,  i.  e,  proprio-ceptive  field.  But  in  the 
instances  given  earlier  the  proprio-ceptive  reflex  which  was 
initiated  secondarily  in  consequence  of  the  foregoing  extero- 
ceptive (skin)  reflex  is  antagonistic  to  the  latter;  the  two 
reflexes  are  related  as  antagonistic  reflexes.  The  secondary 
association  which,  as  pointed  out  earlier,  holds  so  generally  be- 
tween certain  extero-ceptive  proprio-ceptive  pairs  of  reflexes 
and  connects  them,  forms  some  of  its  pairs  from  "  allied  re- 
flexes "  and  others  from  "  antagonistic  reflexes."  In  the  former 
case  the  coupling  is  "  simultaneous,"  in  the  latter  **  successive." 
Proprio-ceptive  reflexes  may  themselves  be  coupled  in  antago- 
nistic pairs ;  of  this,  one  example  is  the  reflex  contraction  of  the 
hamstring  muscles,  which  sometimes  undoubtedly  ensues  on  stim- 
ulation of  the  central  end  of  the  nerve  of  the  extensor  of  the  knee 
(I  was  at  first  inclined  to  attribute  this  to  escape  of  current,  but 
am  convinced  it  occurs  truly  reflexly  sometimes)  ;  and  another 
example  is  Mislawski's  and  Baglioni's  interesting  expiratory 
reflex  elicitable  from  the  afferent  fibres  of  the  phrenic  nerve. 

Factors  determining  the  sequence.     The  formation  of  a  com- 
mon path  from  tributary  converging  afferent  arcs  is  important 


206  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

because  it  gives  a  co-ordinating  mechanism.  There  the  domi- 
nant action  of  one  afferent  arc,  or  set  of  allied  arcs  in  condomin- 
ium, is  subject  to  supercession  by  another  afferent  arc  or  set  of 
allied  arcs,  and  the  supercession  normally  occurs  without  inter- 
current confusion.  Whatever  be  the  nature  of  the  physiological 
process  occurring  between  the  competing  reflexes  for  dominance 
over  the  common  path,  the  issue  of  their  competition,  namely, 
the  determination  of  which  one  of  the  competing  arcs  shall  for 
the  time  being  reign  over  the  common  path,  is  largely  condi- 
tioned by  four  factors.  These  are  "  spinal  induction,"  ^^  rela- 
tive intensity  of  stimulus,  relative  fatigue,  and  the  functional 
species  of  the  reflex. 

I.  Spinal  induction.  The  first  of  these  occurs  in  two  forms, 
one  of  which  has  been  already  considered,  namely,  **  immediate 
induction."  It  is  a  form  of  "  bahnung."  The  stimulus  which 
excites  a  reflex  tends  by  central  spread  to  facilitate  and  lower 
the  threshold  for  reflexes  allied  to  that  which  it  particularly 
excites.  A  constellation  of  reflexes  thus  tends  to  be  formed 
which  reinforce  each  other,  so  that  the  reflex  is  supported  by 
allied  accessory  reflexes,  or  if  the  prepotent  stimulus  shifts, 
allied  arcs  are  by  the  induction  particularly  prepared  to  be 
responsive  to  it  or  to  a  similar  stimulus. 

Immediate  induction  only  occurs  between  allied  reflexes.  Its 
tendency  in  the  competition  between  afferent  arcs  is  to  fortify 
the  reflex  just  established,  or,  if  transition  occur,  to  favour  transi- 
tion to  an  allied  reflex.  Immediate  induction  seen;is  to  obtain 
with  highest  intensity  at  the  outset  of  a  reflex,  or  at  least  near 
its  commencement.     It  does  not  appear  to  persist  long. 

The  other  form  of  spinal  induction  is  what  may  be  termed 
successive  induction.  It  is  in  several  ways  the  reverse  of  the 
preceding.^^ 

In  peripheral  inhibition  exemplified  by  the  vagus  action  on 
the  heart  the  inhibitory  effect  is  followed  by  a  rebound  after- 
effect opposite  to  the  inhibitory  (Gaskell).  The  same  thing 
is  obvious  in  various  instances  of  the  reciprocal  inhibition 
of  the  spinal  centres.  Thus,  if  the  crossed-extension  reflex  of 
the  limb  of  the  spinal   dog   be   elicited   at  regular  intervals. 


VI] 


SUCCESSIVE  INDUCTION 


207 


Q 
U 

< 


X  h  -M  -a  5    •>  « 

«  JS    «   -2  5  Q    g 

*§  9  W  .s  a,  r.  M 

§  §  ^-  2  !  iG  « 

S  tj  >  S  ^  <o  *^ 

_j  0,    ?    rt    c  tc    J" 


;;   ra   u    o 

3       to  Q 

U    ^      CO 


i-Sl 


i_i  s   <" 
S  S  p 


c5^ 


S   o   <u 


^     JJ     S     4, 

C    !?    >- 

o    S  ^ 


(A 

o 


"  '>i 


^  CO  j3  q3  to    S 

^  -^  5S 

j^  2  §  i?  5i 

«  S  «5  2  c  3  ^ 

«c  «  5i  c  «  2  ^ 

^  .5  «  8  5  &| 

S     4>     O  - 


I 


13  o 


2  S  S  ^ 


§1l-5i  = 


«  o   o 


s 


,S  X  cfl  T  ^  M.S 

s?  I  ^  I  s  I  ^ 

C  ^    .       __ 


(«    •.  .5   «   «  .22 


^"S 


a  '55 

3  e  -  fc, 

I  ^  -  ^  I  c  -G 

-^  SI  §  M  ^  '^  ** 

pj    2  rt  c 

p    O  N 


c«   «r>' 


2o8  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect.  ] 

say  once  a  minute,  by  a  carefully  adjusted  electrical  stimulus  \ 

of  defined  duration  and  intensity,  the  resulting  reflex  move-  1 

ments  are  repeated  each  time  with  much  constancy  of  char-  i 

acter,   amplitude,   and   duration.     If  in   one   of   the   intervals  i 

a  strong  prolonged   (e.  g.  30")  flexion-reflex  is  induced  from  1 

the   limb   yielding    the    extensor-reflex   movement,   the   latter  -■ 

reflex  is  found  intensified  after  the  intercurrent  flexion-reflex.*^  > 

The   intercalated    flexion-reflex  lowers   the   threshold    for  the  - 

aftercoming  extension-reflexes,  and   especially   increases   their  \ 

after-discharge  (Figs.  57,  58).     This  effect  may  endure,  pro-  \ 

gressively  diminishing,  through  four  or  five  minutes,  as  tested  i 

by  the  extensor-reflexes  at  successive  intervals.     Now,  as  we  ;] 

have  seen,  durifig  the  flexion-reflex  the   extensor   arcs   were  1 

inhibited:   after  the  flexion-reflex  these  arcs  are  in  this  case  \ 

evidently  in  a  phase  of  exalted  excitability.     The  phenomenon  | 

presents  obvious  analogy  to  visual  contrast.     If  visual  bright-  \ 

ness  be  regarded  as  analogous  to   the  activity  of  spinal  dis-  '\ 

charge,    and  .  visual   darkness   analogous   to   absence  of  spinal  J 

discharge,  this  reciprocal   spinal  action  in  the  example  men-  % 

tioned  has  a  close  counterpart  in  the  well-known  experiment  ■ 

where  a  white  disc  used  as  a  prolonged  stimulus  leaves  as  visual  ^ 

after-effect  a  gray  image  surrounded  by  a  bright  ring  (Hering's  \ 

**  Lichthof ").     The  bright  ring  has  for  its  spinal  equivalent  the  ; 
discharge  from  the  adjacent  reciprocally  correlated  spinal  centre. 

The  exaltation  after-effect  may  ensue  with  such  intensity  that  : 

simple  discontinuance  of  the  stimulus  maintaining  one  reflex  is  \ 

immediately   followed    by  "  spontaneous "    appearance    of  the  ' 

antagonistic  reflex.     Thus  the  "  flexion-reflex  "  if  intense  and  i 

prolonged  may,  directly  its  own  exciting  stimulus  is  discontinued,  ; 
be  succeeded  by  a  "  spontaneous  "  reflex  of  extension,  and  this 

even  when  the  animal  is  lying  on  its  side  and  the  limb  hori-  \ 

zontal,  —  a  pose  that  does  not  favour  the  tonus  of  the  extensor  \ 

muscles.     Such  a  "  spontaneous  "  reflex  is  the  spinal  counter-  \ 

part  of  the  visual  **  Lichthof."     To  this  "  spinal  induction,"  as  i 

it  may  be  termed,  seems  attributable  a  phenomenon  commonly  : 

met  in  a  flexion-reflex  of  high  intensity  when  maintained  by  very  \ 

prolonged    excitation.     The   reflex-flexion   is   then   frequently  ' 


VI] 


SUCCESSIVE  INDUCTION 


209 


Figure  58.  — The  crossed  extension-reflex.  This  reflex  was  being  elicited  regularly  by 
eleven  break  shocks  (unipolar  faradization  to  skin  of  opposite  foot)  at  one-minute  inter- 
vals, stimulus  and  reflex  being  of  low  intensity.  In  the  interval  between  B  and  C  a 
strong  flexion-reflex  of  the  limb  responding  in  the  crossed  extension-reflex  was  provoked 
and  maintained  for  45  seconds.  The  next  following  extension-reflex  C  shows  augmenta- 
tion; this  augmentation  is  also  evident,  though  less  in  the  next  crossed  reflex  D.  In  E, 
a  minute  later,  the  augmentation  is  seen  to  have  passed  ofif.  The  sigjnal  recording  the 
stimulation  evoking  each  crossed  extension-reflex  is  above.  Time  is  marked  in  seconds 
below. 

broken  at  irregular  intervals  by  sudden  extension  movements 
(Figs.  45,  61).  It  would  seem,  therefore,  that  some  process 
in  the  flexion-reflex  leads  to  exaltation  of  the  activity  of  the 
arcs  of  the  opposed  extension-reflex.  And  electrical  stimula- 
tion of  the  proximal  end  of  the  severed  nerve  of  the  extensor 
muscles  of  the  knee  (cat),  though  it  does  not  in  my  expe- 
rience directly  excite  contraction  of  the  extensors  of  the  knee, 
is  on  cessation  often  immediately  followed  by  contraction  of 
them. 


2IO 


REFLEX  SEQUENCE 


[Lect. 


Figure  59.  —  "Mark-time"  reflex  arrested  by  inhibition.  Record  of  movement  of  limb 
(spinal  dog).  The  upstrokes  correspond  with  flexions.  The  reflex  is  interrupted  by 
stimulation  of  the  tail.  This  arrest  is  followed,  after  discontinuance  of  the  inhibitory 
stimulus,  by  increased  amplitude  and  some  quickening  of  the  leg-movement.  Signal  line 
marks  duration  of  inhibitory  stimulus.     Time  above  in  seconds. 


VI] 


SUCCESSIVE  INDUCTION 


211 


Figure  6o.  —  "Mark-time"  reflex  arrested  by  removing  the  exciting  stimulus.  Record  of 
movements  as  before.  During  the  period  between  the  two  marks  on  the  signal  line  the 
reflex  was  interrupted  by  lifting  the  fellow-limb  to  that  yielding  the  tracing.  On  letting 
the  leg  hang  again,  the  reflex  starts  afresh,  but  without  increase  beyond  its  previous 
activity. 


As  examples  of  the  rebound  exaltation  following  on  inhibi- 
tion the  following  may  also  serve.^^  The  so-called  *'  mark-time  " 
reflex  of  the  "  spinal  "  dog  is  an  alternating  stepping  movement 
of  the  hind  limbs  which  occurs  on  holding  the  animal  up  so  that 
its  limbs  hang  pendent.  It  can  be  inhibited  by  stimulating  the 
skin  of  the  tail.  On  cessation  of  that  stimulus  the  stepping 
movement  sets  in  more  vigorously  and  at  quicker  rate  than 
before  (Fig.  59).  The  increase  is  chiefly  in  the  amplitude  of 
the  movement,  but  I  have  also  seen  the  rhythm  quickened  even 
by  30  per  cent  of  the  frequence. 

This  after-increase  might  be  explicable  in  either  of  two  ways. 
It  might  be  due  to  the  mere  repose  of  the  reflex  centre,  the  re- 
pose so  recruiting  the  centre  as  to  strengthen  its  subsequent 
action.     But  a  similar   period    of  repose  obtained    by  simply 


212  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

supporting  one  limb  —  which  causes  cessation  of  the  reflex  in 
both  limbs,  the  stimulus  being  stretch  of  the  hip-flexors  under 
gravity —  is  not  followed  by  after-increase  of  the  reflex  (Fig.  60). 

Or  the  after-increase  might  result  from  the  inhibition  being 
followed  by  a  rebound  to  superactivity.  This  latter  seems  to 
be  the  case.  The  after-increase  occurs  even  when  both  hind 
limbs  are  passively  lifted  from  below  during  the  whole  duration 
of  the  inhibitory  stimulus  applied  to  the  tail.  It  is  the  depres- 
sion of  inhibition  and  not  the  mere  freedom  from  an  exciting 
stimulus  that  induces  a  later  superactivity.  And  the  reflex  in- 
hibition of  the  knee-extensor  by  stimulation  of  the  central  end 
of  its  own  nerve  is  especially  followed  by  marked  rebound  to 
superactivity  of  the  extensor  itself. 

Again,  the  knee-jerk,  after  being  inhibited  by  stimulation  of 
the  hamstring  nerve,  returns,  and  is  then  more  brisk  than  before 
the  inhibition  (Fig.  29,  p.  89). 

'  By  virtue  of  this  spinal  contrast,  therefore,  the  extension- 
reflex  predisposes  to  and  may  actually  induce  a  flexion- 
reflex,  and  conversely  the  flexion-reflex  predisposes  to  and 
may  actually  induce  an  extension-reflex.  This  process  is 
qualified  to  play  a  part  in  linking  reflexes  together  in  a  co- 
ordinate sequence  of  successive  combination.^  If  a  reflex-arc 
A  during  its  own  activity  temporarily  checks  that  of  an  opposed 
reflex-arc  B,  but  as  a  subsequent  result  induces  in  arc  B  a  phase 
of  greater  excitability  and  capacity  for  discharge,  it  predisposes 
the  spinal  organ  for  a  second  reflex  opposite  in  character  to  its 
own  in  immediate  succession  to  itself.  I  have  elsewhere  ^^ 
pointed  out  the  peculiar  prominence  of  **  alternating  reflexes  " 
in  prolonged  spinal  reactions.  It  is  significant  that  they  are 
usually  cut  short  with  ease  by  mere  passive  mechanical  inter- 
ruption of  the  alternating  movement  in  progress.  It  seems 
that  each  step  of  the  reflex  movement  tends  to  excite  by  spinal 
induction  the  step  next  succeeding  itself. 

Much  of  the  reflex  action  of  the  limb  that  can  be  studied  in 
the  "  spinal "  dog  bears  the  character  of  adaptation  to  locomo- 
tion. This  has  been  shown  recently  with  particular  clearness 
by  the  observations  of  Phillipson.     In  describing  the  extensor 


VI]  SUCCESSIVE   INDUCTION  213 

thrust  of  the  limb  I  drew  attention  at  the  time  to  its  signifi- 
cance for  locomotion.  "  Spinal  induction "  obviously  tends 
to  connect  to  this  extensor-thrust  flexion  of  the  limb  as  an 
after-effect.  In  the  stepping  of  the  limb  the  flexion  that  raises 
the  foot  and  carries  it  clear  of  the  ground  prepares  the  antago- 
nistic arcs  of  extension,  and,  so  to  say,  sensitizes  them  to 
respond  later  in  their  turn  by  the  supporting  and  propulsive 
extension  of  the  limb  necessary  for  progression.  In  such  reflex 
sequences  an  antecedent  reflex  would  thus  not  only  be  the  means 
of  bringing  about  an  ensuing  stimulus  for  the  next  reflex,  but 
would  predispose  the  arc  of  the  next  reflex  to  react  to  the 
stimulus  when  it  arrives,  or  even  induce  the  reflex  without  ex- 
ternal stimulus.  The  reflex  "stepping"  of  the  "spinal"  dog 
does  go  on  even  without  an  external  skin  stimulus :  it  will  con- 
tinue when  the  dog  is  held  in  the  air.  The  cat  walks  well  when 
anaesthetic  in  the  soles  of  all  four  feet. 

Each  reflex  movement  must  of  itself  generate  stimuli  to 
afferent  apparatus  in  many  parts  and  organs  —  muscles,  joints, 
tendons,  etc.  This  probably  reinforces  the  reflex  in  progress. 
The  reflex  obtainable  by  stimulation  of  the  afferent  nerve  of  the 
flexor  muscles  of  the  knee  excites  those  muscles  to  contraction 
and  inhibits  their  antagonistics :  the  reflex  obtainable  from  the 
afferent  nerve  of  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  knee  excites  the 
flexors  and  inhibits  their  antagonists. 

Where  a  reflex  by  spinal  induction  tends  to  eventually  bring 
about  the  opposed  reflex,  the  process  of  spinal  induction  is 
therefore  probably  reinforced  by  the  operation  of  any  reflex 
generated  in  the  movement.  This  would  help  to  explain  how 
it  is  that  a  reflex  reaction,  when  once  excited  in  a  spinal  animal, 
ceases  on  cessation  of  the  stimulus  as  quickly  as  it  generally 
does.  Such  a  reaction  must  generate  in  its  progress  a  number 
of  further  stimuli  and  throw  up  a  shower  of  centripetal  im- 
pulses from  the  moving  muscles  and  joints  into  the  spinal 
cord.  Squeezing  of  muscles  and  stimulation  of  their  afferent 
nerves  and  those  of  joints,  etc.,  elicit  reflexes.  The  primary 
reflex  movement  might  be  expected,  therefore,  of  itself  to  ini- 
tiate further  reflex  movement,  and  that  secondarily  to  initiate 


214  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

further  still,  and  so  on.  Yet  on  cessation  of  the  external  stimulus 
to  the  foot  in  the  "  flexion-reflex  "  the  whole  reflex  comes  usually 
at  once  to  an  end.  The  *'  scratch-reflex,"  even  when  violently- 
provoked,  ceases  usually  within  two  seconds  of  the  discontinu- 
ance of  the  external  stimulus  that  provoked  it. 

We  have  as  yet  no  satisfactory  explanation  of  this.  But  we 
remember  that  such  reflexes  are  intercurrent  reactions  breaking 
in  on  a  condition  of  neural  equilibrium  itself  reflex.  The  suc- 
cessive induction  will  tend  to  induce  a  compensatory  reflex, 
which  brings  the  moving  parts  back  again  to  the  original  posi- 
tion of  equilibrium. 

II.  Fatigue.  Another  condition  influencing  the  issue  of  com- 
petition between  reflexes  of  different  source  for  possession  of 
one  and  the  same  final  common  path  is  "  fatigue."  ^^  A  spinal 
reflex  under  continuous  excitation  or  frequent  repetition  be- 
comes weaker,  and  may  cease  altogether.  This  decline  is  pro- 
gressive, and  takes  place  earlier  in  some  kinds  of  reflexes  than 
it  does  in  others.  In  the  spinal  dog  the  scratch-reflex  under 
ordinary  circumstances  tires  much  more  rapidly  (Fig.  45)  than 
does  the  "  flexion-reflex." 

A  reflex  as  it  tires  shows  other  changes  besides  decline  in 
amplitude  of  contraction.  Thus,  in  the  "  flexion-reflex "  the 
original  steadiness  of  the  contraction  decreases  (Figs.  45,  61); 
it  becomes  tremulous,  and  the  tremor  becomes  progressively 
more  marked  aud  more  irregular.  The  rhythm  of  the  tremor 
in  my  observations  has  often  been  about  10  per  second.  Then 
phases  of  greater  tremor  tend  to  alternate  with  phases  of  im- 
proved contraction  as  indicated  by  some  regain  of  original  ex- 
tent of  flexion  of  limb  and  diminished  tremor.  Apart  from  these 
partial  evanescent  recoveries  the  decline  is  progressive.  Later, 
the  stimulation  being  maintained  all  the  time,  brief  periods  of 
something  like  complete  intermission  of  the  reflex  appear,  and 
even  of  a  replacement  of  flexion  by  extension.  These  lapses 
are  recovered  from,  but  tend  to  recur  more  and  more.  Finally, 
an  irregular  phasic  tremor  of  the  muscles  is  all  that  remains. 
It  is  not  the  flexor  muscles  themselves  which  tire  out,  for  these, 
when  under  fatigue  of  the  "  flexion-reflex "  they  contract  no 


VI] 


FATIGUE 


2IS 


Figure  6i.  —  Flexion-reflex.  The  reflex  was  being  elicited  (by  unipolar  faradization  applied 
to  a  point  in  the  plantar  skin  of  the  outermost  digit)  with  intervals  of  about  60  seconds 
between  the  end  of  one  reaction  and  the  commencement  of  the  next.  A  shows  the  com- 
mencement of  the  third  reaction  in  the  series,  and  this  reaction  was  continuously  elicited 
for  50  seconds ;  B  shows  it  in  its  latest  stage.  The  stimulation  was  then  stopped  for 
70  seconds  and  then  recommenced  :  C  shows  the  opening  of  the  next  reaction,  the  fourth 
of  the  series.  In  the  70  seconds  interval  the  reflex  has  fully  recovered  from  the  "  fatigue  " 
exhibited  in  B.  Signal  above  shows  interruptions  in  primary  circuit,  40  per  second. 
Time  below  in  seconds.     An  abscissa  indicates  the  latent  periods  in  A  and  C. 


longer  for  that  reflex,  contract  in  response  to  the  scratch-reflex 
which  also  employs  them. 

Similar  results  are  furnished  by  the  scratch-reflex  with  certain 
differences  in  accord  with  the  peculiar  character  of  its  dis- 
charge.^^*^  One  of  these  latter  is  the  feature  that  the  individual 
beats  of  the  scratch-reflex  usually  become  slower  and  follow 
each   other  at  slower  frequency    (Fig.   62).     Also   the    beats, 


2i6  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 


I 


FiGURK  6a.  —  The  scratch-reflex  evoked  in  spinal  dog  by  mechanical  stimulation  at  a  spot  in 
the  skin  of  the  shoulder.  The  reflex  shows  the  rapid  waning  of  the  movement  under 
continued  application  of  stimuli  to  one  sp>ot  of  skin  —  this  occurs  with  the  same  general 
features  both  under  mechanical  or  electrical  stimulation.  The  beats  become  of  slower 
rhythm  and  more  irregular  and  smaller  amplitude  and  finally  occur  in  groups.  Time  is 
marked  below  in  seconds. 


instead  of  remaining  fairly  regular  in  amplitude  and  frequency 
tend  to  succeed  in  somewhat  regular  groups.  The  beats  may 
disappear  altogether  for  a  short  time,  and  then  for  a  short  time 
reappear,  the  stimulus  continuing  all  the  while  (Fig.  63).  Here, 
again,  the  phenomena  are  not  referable  to  the  muscle,  for  when 
excited  through  other  reflex  channels,  or  through  its  motor- 
nerve  directly,  the  muscle  shows  its  contraction  well.  Part  of 
the  decline  of  these  reflexes  under  electrical  stimulation  in  the 
spinal  dog  may  be  due  to  reduction  of  the  intensity  of  the 
stimulus  itself  by  physical  polarization.  That  does  not  account 
in  the  main  for  the  above  described  effects.  The  graphic 
record  of  fatigue  of  the  flexion  of  the  scratch-reflex  obtained  by 
continued  mechanical  stimulation  does  not  appreciably  differ 
from  that  yielded  under  electrical  stimulation.  The  diff*erent 
speed  of  the  decline  due  to  fatigue  proceeds  characteristically  in 


VI] 


FATIGUE 


217 


Figure  63.  —  The  scratch-reflex  toward  the  end  of  a  Ions-maintained  mechanical  stimulation 
applied  to  a  point  of  the  skin  of  the  shoulder.  The  lowest  signal  line  marks  the  time  of 
application  of  this  stimulation,  which,  when  the  reflex  was  nearly  tired  out,  was  remitted 
for  about  10  seconds  and  then  repeated.  The  reflex  then  returns,  showing  considerable 
reeorery.  The  second  signal  line  from  the  bottom  shows  the  time  of  application  of  a 
nmilar  stimulus  applied  to  a  point  of  skin  two  centimeters  distant  from  the  long-maintained 
one.  The  reflex  elicited  from  that  neighbouring  point  shows  little  evidence  of  fatigue. 
Time  is  marked  above  in  seconds. 


2i8  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

different  kinds  of  reflex,  and  in  the  same  kind  of  reflex  under 
different  physiological  conditions  e.  g.  "  spinal  shock  " :  this  in- 
dicates its  determination  by  other  factors  than  electrical  polar-       ^ 
ization.     Polarization  has  in  a  number  of  cases  been  deferred  as  Si 
far  as  possible  by  using  equalized  alternate  shocks  applied  in 
opposite  directions  through  the  same  gilt  needle ;  this  precau-       i 
tion  has  not  yielded  results  differing  appreciably  from  those 
given  by  ordinary  double  shocks  or  by  series  of  make  or  break 
shocks  of  the   same  direction.     The   slowing   of  the   beat   in 
"  fatigue  "  is  also  against  the  explanation  by  polarization,  since 
merely  weakening   the   stimulus   does    not   lead   to   a   slower 
beat. 

When   the  scratch-reflex   elicited   from   a   spot  of  skin   is 
fatigued,  the  fatigue  holds  for  that  spot  but  does  not  implicate 
the  reflex  as  obtained   from  the   surrounding  skin.^s"*^^    The 
reflex  is  when  tired  out  to  stimuli  at  that  spot  easily  obtainable       ^ 
by  stimulation  two  or  more  centimeters  away  (Fig.  63).     This 
is  seen  with  either  mechanical  or  electrical  stimuli.     When  the 
spot  stimulated  second  is  close  to  the  one  tired  out,  the  reflex 
shows  some  degree  of  fatigue,  but  not  that  degree  obtaining  for 
the  original  spot.     This  fatigue  may  be  a  local  fatigue  of  the 
nerve-endings  in  the  spot  of  skin  stimulated,  to  which  in  experi- 
ments making  use  of  electric  stimuli  some  polarization  may  be       \ 
added.     Yet  its  local  character  does  not  at  all  necessarily  imply 
its  reference  to  the  skin.     It  may  be  the  expression  of  a  spatial 
arrangement  in  the  central  organ  by  which  reflex-arcs  arising  in 
adjacent   receptors   are    partially  confluent   in   their   approach       i 
toward  the  final  common  path,  and  are  the  more  confluent  the 
closer  together  lie  their  points  of  origin  in  the  receptive  field. 
The    resemblance    between  the  distribution    of  the    incidence       ! 
of  this  fatigue  and  that  of  the  spatial  summation  previously 
described  argues  that  the  seat  of  the  fatigue  is  intraspinal  and      ^ 
central  more  than  peripheral  and  cutaneous ;  and  that  it  affects       * 
the  afferent  part  of  the  arc  inside  the  spinal  cord,  probably  at 
the  first  synapse.     Thus,  its  incidence  at  the  synapse  Ra  —  Pa       , 
and  at  RyS  — PyS  (Fig.  13,  B,  or  39,  B)  would  explain  its  restric-     | 
tions,  as  far  as  we  know  them,  in  the  scratch-reflex. 


VI]  FATIGUE  219 

The  local  fatigue  of  a  spinal  reflex  seems  to  be  recovered 
from  with  remarkable  speed,  to  judge  by  observations  on  the 
reflexes  of  the  limbs  of  the  spinal  dog.  A  few  seconds'  re- 
mission of  the  stimulus  suffices  for  marked  though  incom- 
plete restoration  of  the  reaction  (Fig.  63).  In  a  few  instances 
I  have  seen  return  of  a  reflex  even  during  the  stimulation 
under  which  the  waning  and  disappearance  of  the  reflex 
occurred.  The  exciting  stimulus  has  usually  in  such  cases 
been  of  rather  weak  intensity.  In  my  experience,  these  spinal 
reflexes  fade  out  sooner  under  a  weak  stimulus  than  under  a 
strong  one.  This  seeming  paradox  indicates  that  under  even 
feeble  intensities  of  stimulation  the  threshold  of  the  reaction 
gradually  rises,  and  that  it  rises  above  the  threshold  value  of 
the  weaker  stimulus  before  it  reaches  that  of  a  stronger  stimulus. 
This  is  exemplified  by  Fig.  64,  where  the  scratch-reflex  which 


Figure  64.  — The  scratch-reflex  evoked  by  a  relatively  feeble  stimulation  and  disappearing 
under  that  stimulation.  On  increasing  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  the  reflex  reappears, 
and  does  not  reappear  on  reverting  again  to  the  original  intensity  of  stimulation.  The 
signal  line  at  top  marks  the  stimulation  by  an  electromagnet  in  the  primary :  the  arma- 
ture is  arranged  so  that  the  increase  in  intensity  of  the  stimulation  is  shown  by  greater 
amplitude  of  excursion.     Time  in  seconds  below. 


220                           REFLEX  SEQUENCE                    [Lect.  ] 

has  ceased  to  be  elicited  by  the  stimulus  A  is  immediately  ). 

evoked  —  often  without  any  sign  of  fatigue  in  its  motor  response  { 

—  by  increasing  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  (applied  at  the  | 

same  electrode)  to  A  +  a,  5  ohms  having  been  short-circuited  ^ 

from  the  current  in  the  primary  circuit.     But  the  occurrence  of  : 

"fatigue"   earlier  under  the  weaker  stimulus  than  under   the  | 

stronger   also    shows   that   the   fatigue   consequent   under   the  \ 

weaker  stimulus  may  often  be  relatively  to  the  production  of  \ 

the  natural  discharge  greater  than  when  a  stronger  stimulus  is  j 

employed.     This  which  has  been  of  frequent  occurrence  in  my  \ 

observations  on  the  leg  of  the  spinal  dog  if  obtaining  widely  in  | 

reflex  actions  has  evident  practical  importance.  | 

It  is  easy  to  avoid  in  some  degree  the  local  fatigue  associated  I 

with  excitation  of  the  scratch-reflex  from  one  single  spot  in  the  ^ 


Figure  65.  —  A  scratch-reflex  reappearing  after  having  lapsed  completely  under  a  stimulation 
(unipolar  faradization  by  double-induction  shocks)  which  has  been  maintained  unaltered 
although  the  reflex  it  originally  evoked  had  lapsed.  The  reappearances  occur  at  irregular 
intervals  of  long  duration,  e.g.  50  seconds,  and  the  reflex  on  reappearance  may  last  for  20 
seconds  at  a  time.  The  stimulation  was  applied  at  ten  separate  points  in  the  skin  surface 
and  at  each  point  by  unipolar  faradization  from  a  separate  secondary  circuit. 

skin  by  taking  advantage  of  the  spatial  summation  of  stimuli 
applied  at  different  points  in  the  receptive  field.  Brief-lasting 
stimuli  can  be  shifted  from  point  to  point  in  the  field.  When 
this  is  done,  a  curious  result  has  met  me.  The  provocation  of 
the  reflex  has  been  made  through  ten  separate  points  in  the 
receptive  field,  the  distance  between  each  member  of  the  series 


VI]  FATIGUE  221 

of  points  and  the  point  next  to  it  being  about  four  centimeters. 
Each  point  is  stimulated  by  a  double-induction  shock  delivered 
twice  a  second.  When  this  is  done  a  series  of  scratch  move- 
ments is  elicited,  and  continues  longer  than  when  the  stimuli 
i-e  applied  at  the  same  interval,  not  to  succeeding  series  of 
:ln  points  but  to  one  point.  Thus  three  or  four  hundred 
;ats  can  be  elicited  in  unbroken  series.  But  the  series  tends 
►mewhat  abruptly  to  cease.  If  then,  in  spite  of  the  ces- 
ition  of  the  response,  the  stimulation  be  continued  without 
teration  during  three  or  four  minutes  or  more,  the  scratch- 
ing movement  breaks  out  again  from  time  to  time  and  gives 
another  series  of  beats  (Fig.  65),  perhaps  longer  than  the  first. 
These  experiments  indicate  that  physical  polarization  at  the 
stigmatic  electrode  is  not  answerable  for  the  fading  out  of  the 
scratch-reflex.  It  shows  also  the  complexity  of  the  central 
mechanisms  involved  in  the  reflex.  The  phenomenon  recalls 
Lombard's  ^^^  phases  of  briskness  and  fatigue  in  series  of  records 
obtained  with  the  ergograph. 

It  is  interesting  to  note  certain  differences  between  the 
cessation  of  a  reflex  under  "  fatigue "  and  under  inhibition. 
Figs.  60  and  43  can  be  compared.  The  reflex  ceasing  under 
inhibition  is  seen  to  fade  off  without  obvious  change  in  the  fre- 
quency of  repetition  of  the  **  beats,"  or  in  the  duration  of  the 
individual  beats.  The  reflex  ceasing  under  fatigue  is  seen  to 
show  a  slower  rhythm  and  a  sluggish  course  for  the  latter  beats, 
especially  for  the  terminal  ones. 

Among  the  signs  of  fatigue  of  a  reflex  action  are  several 
suggesting  that  in  it  the  command  over  the  final  common  path 
exercised  for  the  time  being  by  the  receptors  and  afferent  path 
in  action  becomes  less  strong,  less  steady,  and  less  accurately 
adjusted.  Under  prolonged  excitation  their  hold  upon  the  final 
common  path  becomes  loosened.  This  view  is  supported  by 
the  fact  that  its  connection  with  the  final  common  path  is  then 
more  easily  cut  short  and  ruptured  by  other  rival  arcs  compet- 
ing with  it  for  the  final  common  path  in  question.  The  scratch- 
reflex  interrupts  the  flexion-reflex  more  readily  when  the  latter 
is  tired  out  than  when  it  is  fresh. 


222  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

In  the  hind  limb  of  the  spinal  dog  the  extensor-thrust  is 
inelicitable  during  the  flexion-reflex.  That  is  to  say,  when  the 
flexion-reflex  is  evoked  with  fair  or  high  intensity  I  have  never 
succeeded  in  evoking  the  extensor-thrust,  though  the  flexed 
posture  of  the  limb  is  itself  a  favouring  circumstance  for  the 
production  of  the  thrust  if  the  flexion  be  a  passive  one.  But 
when  the  flexion-reflex  is  kept  up  by  appropriate  stimulation  of 
a  single  point  over  a  prolonged  time,  so  that  it  shows  fatigue,  the 
"extensor-thrust"  becomes  again  elicitable.  Its  elicitabih'ty  is, 
then,  not  regular  nor  facile,  but  it  does  become  obtainable, 
usually  in  quite  feeble  degree  at  first,  later  more  powerfully.  In 
other  words,  it  can  dispossess  the  rival  reflex  from  a  common 
path  when  that  rival  is  fatigued,  though  it  cannot  do  so  when  the 
rival  action  is  fresh  and  powerful. 

Again,  the  crossed  **  extension-reflex "  cannot  inhibit  the 
flexion  of  the  flexor-reflex  under  ordinary  circumstances  if  the 
intensity  of  the  stimulation  of  the  competing  arcs  be  approxi- 
mately equal ;  but  it  can  do  so  when  the  flexion-reflex  is  tired. 

The  waning  of  a  reflex  under  long  maintained  excitation  is 
one  of  the  many  phenomena  that  pass  in  physiology  under  the 
name  of  "  fatigue."  It  may  be  that  in  this  case  the  so-called 
fatigue  is  really  nothing  but  a  negative  induction.  Its  place  of 
incidence  may  lie  at  the  synapse.  It  seems  a  process  elaborated 
and  preserved  in  the  selective  evolution  of  the  neural  machin- 
ery. One  obvious  use  attaching  to  it  is  the  prevention  of  the 
too  prolonged  continuous  use  of  a  "  common  path  "  by  any  one 
receptor.^  It  precludes  one  receptor  from  occupying  for  long 
periods  an  efiector  organ  to  the  exclusion  of  all  other  receptors. 
It  prevents  long  continuous  possession  of  a  common  path  by 
any  one  reflex  of  considerable  intensity.  It  favours  the  receptors 
taking  turn  about.  It  helps  to  insure  serial  variety  of  reaction. 
The  organism,  to  be  successful  in  a  million-sided  environment, 
must  in  its  reactions  be  many-sided.  Were  it  not  for  such  so- 
called  "  fatigue,"  an  organism  might,  in  regard  to  its  receptivity, 
develop  an  eye,  or  an  ear,  or  a  mouth,  or  a  hand  or  leg,  but  it 
would  hardly  develop  the  marvellous  congeries  of  all  those  vari- 
ous sense-organs  which  it  is  actually  found  to  possess. 


VI]  INTENSITY  223 

The  loosening  of  the  hold  upon  the  common  path  by  so- 
called  "fatigue"  occurs  also  in  paths  other  than  those  leading 
to  muscle  and  effector  organs.  If  instead  of  motor  effects  sen- 
sual are  examined,  analogous  phenomena  are  observed.  A 
visual  image  is  more  readily  inhibited  by  a  competing  image  in 
the  same  visual  field  when  it  has  acted  for  some  time  than  when 
it  is  first  perceived  (W.  Macdougall).^^^ 

One  point,  on  a  priori  grounds  a  natural  corollary  from  the 
"  principle  of  the  common  path,"  is  indicated  by  the  experi- 
mental findings  relative  to  the  incidence  of  fatigue.  The  reflex- 
arcs,  each  a  chain  of  neurones,  converge  in  their  course  so  as  to 
impinge  upon  and  conjoin  in  links  (neurones)  common  to  whole 
varied  groups  —  in  other  words,  they  conjoin  to  common  paths. 
This  arrangement  culminates  in  the  convergence  of  many  sepa- 
rately arising  arcs  in  the  final  efferent-root  neurone.  This  neu- 
rone thus  forms  the  instrument  for  many  different  reflex  arcs 
and  acts.  It  is  responsive  to  them  in  various  rhythm  and  in 
various  grades  of  intensity.  In  accordance  with  this,  it  seems 
from  experimental  evidence  to  be  relatively  indefatigable?^ 
It  thus  satisfies  a  demand  that  the  principle  of  the  common  path 
must  make  regarding  it. 

III.  Intensity.  In  the  transition  from  one  reflex  to  another 
a  final  common  path  changes  hands  and  passes  from  one  master 
to  another.  A  fresh  set  of  afferent  arcs  becomes  dominant  on 
the  supercession  of  one  reflex  by  the  next.  Of  all  the  condi- 
tions determining  which  one  of  competing  reflexes  shall  for  the 
time  being  reign  over  a  final  common  path,  the  intensity  of  re- 
action of  the  afferent  arc  itself  relatively  to  that  of  its  rivals 
is  probably  the  most  powerful.  An  afferent  arc  strongly  stimu- 
lated is  caeteris  paribus  more  likely  to  capture  the  common  path 
than  is  one  excited  feebly.  A  stimulus  can  only  establish  its 
reflex  and  inhibit  an  opposed  one  if  it  have  intensity.  This 
explains  why,  in  order  to  produce  examples  of  spinal  inhibi- 
tion, recourse  has  so  frequently  been  made  in  past  times  to 
strong  stimuli.  A  strong  stimulus  will  inhibit  a  reflex  in  progress, 
although  a  weak  one  will  fail.  Thus  in  Goltz's  inhibition  of  mic- 
turition in  the  "  spinal"  dog  2l  forcible  squeeze  of  the  tail  will  do 


1 

224  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect.  : 

it,  but  not,  in  my  experience,  a  weak  squeeze.  So  likewise  any  '| 
condition  which  raises  the  excitability  and  responsiveness  of  a  ' 
nervous  arc  will  give  it  power  to  inhibit  other  reflexes  just  as  it  'i 
would  if  it  were  excited  by  a  strong  stimulus.  This  is  much  as  j 
in  the  heart  of  the  Tunicate.  There  the  prepotent  spot  whence  t 
starts  the  systole  lies  from  time  to  time  at  one  end  and  from  l 
time  to  time  at  the  other.  The  prepotent  region  at  one  end  | 
which  usually  dominates  the  common  path  is  from  time  to  time  i 
displaced  by  local  increase  of  excitability  at  the  other  under  j 
local  distension  of  the  blood-sinuses  there.  f 

In  judging  of  intensity  of  stimulus  the  situation  of  the  i 
stimulus  in  the  receptive  field  of  the  reflex  has  to  be  remem-  [; 
bered.  One  and  the  same  physical  stimulus  will  be  weak  if  | 
applied  near  the  edge  of  the  field,  though  strong  if  applied  to  t] 
the  focus  of  the  field.  ^ 

Crossed  reflexes  are  usually  less  easy  to  provoke,  less  reliable  | 
of  obtainment,  and  less  intense  than  are  direct  reflexes.  Conse-  ^ 
quently  we  find  crossed  reflexes  usually  more  easily  inhibited  .• 
and  replaced  by  direct  reflexes  than  are  these  latter  by  those  i 
former.  Thus  the  crossed  stepping-reflex  is  easily  replaced  by  ■ 
the  scratch-reflex  (Fig.  52),  though  its  stimulus  be  continued  all  1 
the  time,  and  though  the  scratch-reflex  itself  is  not  a  very  • 
potent  reflex.  But  the  reverse  c^  occur  with  suitably  adjusted  j 
intensity  of  stimuli. 

Again,  the  flexion-reflex  of  the  dog's  leg  is,  when  fully  devel-  ^ 
oped,  accompanied  by  extension  in  the  opposite  leg.  This  : 
crossed  extensor  movement,  though  often  very  vigorous,  may  be 
considered  as  an  accessory  and  weaker  part  of  the  whole  reflex  ^ 
of  which  the  prominent  part  is  flexion  of  the  homonymous  limb.  ] 
When  the  flexion-reflex  is  elicitable  poorly,  as,  for  instance,  in  ; 
spinal  shock  or  under  fatigue  or  weak  excitation,  the  crossed  ex-  ; 
tension  does  not  accompany  the  homonymous  flexion  and  does  i 
not  appear.  But,  where  the  flexion-reflex  is  well  developed,  if  : 
not  merely  one  but  dotk  feet  be  stimulated  simultaneously  with  ; 
stimuli  of  fairly  equal  intensity,  steady  flexion  at  knee,  hip,  and  i 
ankle  results  in  dotA  limbs  (Fig.  66}  and  extension  occurs  in  ! 
neither   limb.^^    The  contralateral   part  of  each  reflex  is   in-  j 


VI]         PREPOTENCY   AND   REFLEX   SPECIES  225 

I  2  3 


Figure  66.  —  Diagram  (cat)  of  the  predominant  uncrossed  flexor-reflex  of  the  hind  limb  in- 
hibiting the  crossed  extensor-reflex  otherwise  obtainable  by  stimulation  of  the  opposite 
limb.  I.  The  initial  pose  of  the  spinal  animal ;  2.  The  pose  assumed  after  stimulation 
of  the  left  hind  foot,  the  flexors  of  the  left  hip,  knee,  and  ankle,  and  the  extensors  of  the 
right  hip,  knee,  and  ankle  are  in  active  contraction ;  3.  The  pose  assumed  after  simulta- 
neous stimulation  of  both  hind  feet.  The  extensor  action  of  the  hip,  knee,  and  ankle 
that  would  appear  from  either  side  as  a  crossed  reflex  is  bilaterally  inhibited  and  the  an- 
tagonistic flexor-reflexes  bilaterally  prevail. 

hibited  by  the  homolateral  flexion  of  each  reflex.  In  other 
words,  the  more  intense  part  of  ^ach  reflex  obtains  possession  of 
the  final  common  paths  at  the  expense  of  the  less  intense  portion 
of  the  reflex.  But  if  the  intensity  of  the  stimuli  applied  to  the 
right  and  left  feet  be  not  closely  enough  balanced,  the  crossed 
extension  of  the  reflex  excited  by  the  stronger  stimulus  is  found 
to  exclude  even  the  homonymous  flexion  that  the  weaker  stimu- 
lus should  and  would  otherwise  evoke  from  the  leg  to  which  it 
is  applied. 

It  was  pointed  out  above  that  in  a  number  of  cases  the 
transference  of  control  of  the  final  common  path  FC  from  one 
afferent  arc  to  another  is  reversible.  The  direction  of  the  trans- 
ference can  caeteris  paribus  be  easily  governed  by  making  the 
stimulation  of  this  receptor  or  that  receptor  the  more  intense. 
A  factor  largely  determining  whether  a  reflex  succeed  another 
or  not  is  therefore  intensity  of  stimulus.^^ 

15 


226  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect.i 

IV.  Species  of  reflex.  A  fourth  main  determinant  for  thei 
issue  of  the  conflict  between  rival  reflexes  seems  the  functional | 
species  of  the  reflexes.^^ 

Reflexes  initiated  from  a  species  of  receptor  apparatus  that] 
may  be  termed  ^^  noci-ceptive'' '^'^  appear  to  particularly  domi-j 
nate  the  majority  of  the  final  common  paths  issuing  from  the 
spinal  cord.  In  the  simpler  sensations  we  experience  from ! 
various  kinds  of  stimuli  applied  to  our  skin  there  can  be : 
distinguished  those  of  touch,  of  cold,  of  warmth,  and  of  pain,  i 
The  adequate  stimuli  for  the  first  mentioned  three  of  these  are' 
certainly  different ;  mechanical  stimuH,  applied  above  a  certain; 
speed,  which  deform  beyond  a  certain  degree  the  resting  con-l 
tour  of  the  skin  surface,  seem  to  constitute  adequate  stimuli  for| 
touch.  Similarly  the  cooling  or  raising  of  the  local  tempera-^ 
ture,  whether  by  thermal  conduction,  radiation,  etc.,  are  ade-^\ 
quate  for  the  cold  and  warmth  sensations.  The  organs  for- 
these  three  sensations  have  by  stigmatic  stimuli  been  traced  to.'i 
separate  and  discrete  tiny  spots  in  the  skin.  In  regard  to  skin-  i 
pain  it  is  held  by  competent  observers,  notably  by  v.  Frey^^! 
and  Kiesow,^'^  that  skin-pain  likewise  is  referable  to  certain 
specific  nerve-endings.  In  evidence  of  this  it  is  urged  thatj 
mechanical  stimuli  applied  at  certain  places  excite  sensations! 
which  from  their  very  threshold  upward  possess  unpleasantness,  < 
and  as  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  is  increased,  culminate  inj 
"physical  pain."  The  sensation  excited  by  a  mechanical^ 
stimulus  applied  to  a  touch-spot  does  not  evoke  pain,  however* 
intensely  applied,  so  long  as  the  stimulation  is  confined  to  the^ 
touch-spot.  The  threshold  value  of  mechanical  stimuH  for* 
touch-spots  is  in  general  lower  than  it  is  for  pain-spots;  and? 
conversely  the  threshold  value  of  electrical  stimuli  for  touch- ^ 
spots  is  in  general  higher  than  it  is  for  the  spots  yielding  pain..: 
Similarly  it  is  said  that  stimulation  of  a  cold  spot  or  of  a  warm.^ 
spot  does  not,  however  intense,  evoke,  so  long  as  confined  to^ 
them,  sensations  of  painful  quality.  But  pain  can  be  excited!^ 
not  only  by  strong  mechanical  stimuli  and  by  electrical  stimuli,^ 
but  by  cold  and  by  warmth,  though  the  threshold  value  o^ 
these  latter  stimuli  is  higher  for  pain  than  for  cold  and  warmt^ 


VI]  NOCI-CEPTIVE   REFLEXES  227 

spots.  If  these  observations  prove  correct  there  exist,  there- 
fore, numerous  specific  cutaneous  nerve-fibres  evoking  pain. 

A  difficulty  here  is  that  sensory  nerve-endings  are  usually 
provided  with  sense-organs  which  lower  their  threshold  for 
stimuli  of  one  particular  kind  while  raising  it  for  stimuli  of  all 
other  kinds;  but  these  pain-endings  in  the  skin  seem  almost 
equally  excited  by  stimuli  of  such  different  modes  as  mechan- 
ical, thermal  conductive,  thermal  radiant,  chemical,  and  elec- 
trical. That  is,  they  appear  anelective  receptors.  But  it  is  to  be 
remarked  that  these  agents,  regarded  as  excitants  of  skin-pain, 
have  all  a  certain  character  in  common,  namely  this,  that  they 
become  adequate  as  excitants  of  pain  when  they  are  of  such  in- 
tensity as  threatens  damage  to  the  skin.  And  we  may  note  about 
these  excitants  that  they  are  all  able  to  excite  nerve  when  applied 
to  naked  nerve  directly.  Now  there  are  certain  skin  surfaces 
from  which,  according  to  most  observers,  pain  is  the  only  species 
of  sensation  that  can  be  evoked.  This  is  alleged,  for  instance, 
of  the  surface  of  the  cornea  —  a  modified  piece  of  skin.  The 
histology  of  the  cornea  jeveals  in  its  epithelium  nerve-endings 
of  but  one  morphological  kind ;  that  is,  the  ending  by  naked 
nerve-fibrils  that  pass  up  among  the  epithelial  cells.  Similar 
nerve-endings  exist  also  in  the  epidermis  generally.  It  may 
therefore  be  that  the  nerve-endings  subserving  skin-pain  are 
free  naked  nerve-endings,  and  the  absence  of  any  highly  evolved 
specialized  end-organ  in  connection  with  them  may  explain 
their  fairly  equal  amenability  to  an  unusually  wide  range  of 
different  kinds  of  stimuli.  Instead  of  but  one  kind  of  stimulus 
being  their  adequate  excitant,  they  may  be  regarded  as  adapted 
to  a  whole  group  of  excitants,  a  group  of  excitants  which  has 
in  relation  to  the  organism  one  feature  common  to  all  its  com- 
ponents, namely,  a  nocuous  character. 

With  its  liability  to  various  kinds  of  mechanical  and  other 
damage  in  a  world  beset  with  dangers  amid  which  the  individual 
and  species  have  to  win  their  way  in  the  struggle  for  existence 
we  may  regard  nocuous  stimuli  as  part  of  a  normal  state  of 
affairs.  It  does  not  seem  improbable,  therefore,  that  there 
should  under  selective  adaptation  attach  to  the  skin  a  so-to-say 


228  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

specific  sense  of  its  own  injuries.  As  psychical  adjunct  to  the 
reactions  of  that  apparatus  we  find  a  strong  displeasurable  affec- 
tive quality  in  the  sensations  they  evoke.  This  may  perhaps 
be  a  means  for  branding  upon  memory,  of  however  rudimen- 
tary kind,  a  feeling  from  past  events  that  have  been  perilously 
critical  for  the  existence  of  the  individuals  of  the  species.  In 
other  words,  if  we  admit  that  damage  to  such  an  exposed  sentient 
organ  as  the  skin  must  in  the  evolutionary  history  of  animal 
life  have  been  sufficiently  frequent  in  relation  to  its  importance, 
then  the  existence  of  a  specific  set  of  nerves  for  skin-pain  seems 
to  offer  no  genetic  difficulty,  any  more  than  does  the  clotting 
of  blood  or  innate  immunity  to  certain  diseases.  That  these 
nerve-endings  constitute  a  distinct  species  is  argued  by  their 
all  evoking  not  only  the  same  species  of  sensation  but  the  same 
species  of  reflex  movement  as  regards  "  purpose,"  intensity,  re- 
sistence  to  "  shock,"  etc.  And  their  evolution  may  well  have 
been  unaccompanied  by  evolution  of  any  specialized  end-organ, 
since  the  naked  free  nerve-endings  would  better  suit  the  wide 
and  peculiar  range  of  stimuli,  reaction  to  which  is  in  this  case 
required.  A  low  threshold  was  not  required  because  the  stimuli 
were  all  intense,  intensity  constituting  their  harmfulness ;  but  re- 
sponse to  a  wide  range  of  stimuH  of  different  kinds  was  required, 
because  harm  might  come  in  various  forms.  That  responsive 
range  is  supplied  by  naked  nerve  itself  and  would  be  cramped 
by  the  specialization  of  an  end-organ.  Hence  these  nerve- 
endings  remained  free. 

It  is  those  areas  stimulation  of  which,  as  judged  by  analogy, 
can  excite  pain  most  intensely,  and  it  is  those  stimuli  which, 
as  judged  by  analogy,  are  most  fitted  to  excite  pain  which, 
as  a  general  rule,  excite  in  the  **  spinal "  animal  —  where  pain  is 
of  course  non-existent  —  the  prepotettt  reflexes.  If  these  are 
reactions  to  specific  pain-nerves,  this  may  be  expressed  by 
saying  that  the  nervous  arcs  of  pain-nerves,  broadly  speaking, 
dominate  the  spinal  centres  in  peculiar  degree.  Physical  pain 
is  thus  the  psychical  adjunct  of  an  imperative  protective  reflex. 
It  is  preferable,  however,  since  into  the  merely  spinal  and  re- 
flex aspect  of  the  reaction  of  these  nerves  no  sensation  of  any 


VI]  SPECIES  OF   REFLEX  229 


L     St 

It 


kind  can  be  shown  to  enter,  to  avoid  the  term  "  pain-nerves." 
Remembering  that  the  feature  common  to  all  this  group  of 
stimuli  is  that  they  threaten  or  actually  commit  damage  to  the 
ssue  to  which  they  are  applied,  a  convenient  term  for  appli- 
ation  to  them  is  nocuous.  In  that  case  what  from  the  point 
of  view  of  sense  are  cutaneous  pain-nerves  are  from  the  point  of 

Iiew  of  reflex  reaction  conveniently  termed  noci-ceptive  nerves. 
In  the  competition  between  reflexes  the  noci-ceptive  as 
rule  dominate  with  peculiar  certainty  and  facility.  This  ex- 
lains  why  such  stimuli  have  been  so  much  used  to  evoke 
:flexes  in  the  spinal  frog,  and  why,  judging  from  them,  such 
fatality  "  belongs  to  spinal  reflexes. 
One  and  the  same  skin  surface  will  in  the  hind  limb  of 
the  spinal  dog  evoke  one  or  other  of  two  diametrically  differ- 
ent reflexes  according  as  the  mechanical  stimulus  applied  be  of 
noxious  quality  or  not,  a  harmful  insult  or  a  harmless  touch.^^ 
A  needle-prick  to  the  planta  causes  invariably  the  drawing 
up  of  the  hmb  —  the  flexion-reflex.  A  harmless  smooth 
contact,  on  the  other  hand,  causes  extension  —  the  extensor- 
thrust  above  described.  This  flexion  is  therefore  a  noci-ceptive 
reflex.  But  the  scratch-reflex  —  which  is  so  readily  evoked 
by  simple  light  irritation  of  the  skin  of  the  shoulder  —  is 
relatively  mildly  noci-ceptive.  When  the  scratch-reflex  and 
the  flexion-reflex  are  in  competition  for  the  final  neurone  com- 
mon to  them,  the  flexion-reflex  more  easily  dispossesses  the 
scratch-reflex  from  the  final  neurone  than  does  the  scratch- 
reflex  the  flexion-reflex.  If  both  reflexes  are  fresh,  and  the 
stimuli  used  are  such  as,  when  employed  separately,  evoke 
their  reflexes  respectively  with  some  intensity,  in  my  experi- 
ence it  is  the  flexion-reflex  that  is  usually  prepotent  (Fig, 
43).  Yet  if,  while  the  flexion-reflex  is  being  moderately  evoked 
by  an  appropriate  stimulus  of  weak  intensity,  a  strong  stimu- 
lus suitable  for  producing  the  scratch-reflex  is  applied,  the 
steady  flexion  due  to  the  flexion-reflex  is  replaced  by  the 
rhythmic  scratching  movement  of  the  scratch-reflex  (Fig.  51), 
and  this  occurs  though  the  stimulus  for  the  flexion-reflex 
is   maintained   unaltered.     When   the   stimulus   producing  the 


230  REFLEX  SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

scratch  is  discontinued  the  flexion-reflex  reappears  as  before. 
The  flexion-reflex  seems  more  easily  to  dispossess  the  scratch- 
reflex  from  the  final  common  paths  than  can  the  scratch-reflex 
dispossess  the  flexion-reflex.  Yet  the  relation  is  reversible  — 
by  heightening  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  for  the  scratch- 
reflex  or  lowering  that  of  the  stimulus  for  the  flexion-reflex. 

In  decerebrate  rigidity,  where  a  tonic  reflex  is  maintaining 
contraction  in  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  knee,  stimulation  of 
the  noci-ceptive  arcs  of  the  limb  easily  breaks  down  that  reflex. 
The  noci-ceptive  reflex  dominates  the  motor  neurone  previ- 
ously held  in  activity  by  the  postural  reflex.  And  noci-ceptive 
reflexes  are  relatively  Httle  depressed  by  "  spinal  shock." 

Noci-ceptive  arcs  are,  however,  not  the  only  spinal  arcs  which 
in  the  intact  animal,  considered  from  the  point  of  view  of  sensa- 
tion, evoke  reactions  rich  in  aff*ective  quality.  Beside  those 
receptors  attuned  to  react  to  direct  noxay  the  skin  has  others, 
concerned  likewise  with  functions  of  vital  importance  to  the 
species  and  colligate  with  sensations  similarly  of  intense  affective 
quality ;  for  instance,  those  concerned  with  sexual  functions.  In 
the  male  frog  sexual  clasp  is  a  spinal  reflex.®  The  cord  may 
be  divided  both  in  front  and  behind  the  brachial  region  without 
interrupting  the  reflex.  Experiment  shows  that  from  the  spinal 
male  at  the  breeding-season,  and  also  at  other  times,  this  reflex 
is  elicited  by  any  object  that  stimulates  the  skin  of  the  sternal 
and  adjacent  region.  In  the  intact  animal,  on  the  contrary, 
other  objects  than  the  female*^  are,  when  applied  to  that  re- 
gion, at  once  rejected,  even  though  they  be  wrapped  in  the  fresh 
skin  of  the  female  frog  and  in  other  ways  made  to  resemble 
the  female.  The  development  of  the  reflex  is  not  prevented  by 
removal  of  the  testes,  but  removal  of  the  seminal  reservoirs  is 
said  to  depress  it,  and  their  distension,  even  by  indifferent  fluids, 
to  exalt  it.  If  the  skin  of  the  sternal  region  and  arms  is  re- 
moved, the  reflex  does  not  occur.  Severe  mutilation  of  the 
limbs  and  internal  organs  does  not  inhibit  the  reflex,  neither 
does  stimulation  of  the  sciatic  nerve  central  to  its  section.  The 
reflex  is  however  depressed  or  extinguished  by  strong  chemical 
and  pathic  stimuli  to  the  sternal  skin,  at  least  in  many  cases. 


PREPOTENT  REFLEXES  231 

The   tortoise   exhibits  a  similiar  sexual  reflex  of  great  spinal 
potency.ii^a   ii2b 

It  would  seem  a  general  rule  that  reflexes  arising  in  species 
of  receptors  which  considered  as  sense-organs  provoke  strongly 
affective  sensation  caeteris  paribus  prevail  over  reflexes  of  other 
species  when  in  competition  with  them  for  the  use  of  the  ''filial 
common  path''  Such  reflexes  override  and  set  aside  with 
peculiar  facility  reflexes  belonging  to  touch  organs,  muscular 
sense-organs,  etc.  As  the  sensations  evoked  by  these  arcs, 
e.g,  "pains,"  exclude  and  dominate  concurrent  sensations,  so 
do  the  reflexes  of  these  arcs  prevail  in  the  competition  for  pos- 
session of  the  common  paths.  They  seem  capable  oi pre-eminent 
intensity  of  action.^^ 

Of  all  reflexes  it  is  the  tonic  reflexes,  e.  g.  of  ordinary 
posture,  that  are  in  my  experience  the  most  easily  interrupted 
by  other  reflexes.  Even  a  weak  stimulation  of  the  noci- 
ceptive arcs  arising  in  the  foot  often  suffices  to  lower  or  abolish 
the  knee-jerk  or  the  reflex  extensor  tonus  of  the  elbow  or 
knee.  If  various  species  of  reflex  are  arranged,  therefore,  in 
their  order  of  potency  in  regard  to  power  to  interrupt  one  an- 
other, the  reflexes  initiated  in  receptors  which  considered  as 
sense-organs  excite  sensations  of  strong  affective  quality  lie  at 
the  upper  end  of  the  scale,  and  the  reflexes  that  are  answerable 
for  the  postural  tonus  of  skeletal  muscles  lie  at  the  lower  end 
of  the  scale.  One  great  function  of  the  tonic  reflexes  is  to 
maintain  habitual  attitudes  and  postures.  They  form,  therefore, 
a  nervous  background  of  active  equilibrium.  It  is  of  obvious 
advantage  that  this  equilibrium  should  be  easily  upset,  so  that 
the  animal  may  respond  agilely  to  the  passing  events  that  break 
upon  it  as  intercurrent  stimuli. 

Therefore,  intensity  of  stimulation,  fatigue  and  freshness, 
spinal  induction,  functional  species  of  reflex,  all  these  are  phys- 
iological factors  influencing  the  result  of  the  interaction  of 
reflex-arcs  at  a  common  path.  It  is  noticeable  that  they  all 
resolve  themselves  ultimately  into  intensity  of  reaction.  Thus, 
intensity  of  stimulus  means  as  a  rule  intensity  of  reaction. 
Those   species   of  reflexes  which  are  habitually  prepotent  in 


232  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect, 

interaction  with  others  are  those  which  are  habitually  intense; 
those  specially  impotent  in  competition  are  those  habitually 
feeble  in  intensity,  e.  g.y  skeletal  muscular  tone.  The  tonic  re-^ 
flexes  of  attitude  are  of  habitually  low  intensity,  easily  interfered 
with  and  temporarily  suppressed  by  intercurrent  reflexes,  these 
latter  having  higher  intensity.  But  these  latter  suffer  fatigue 
relatively  early,  whereas  the  tonic  reflexes  of  posture  can  per- 
sist hour  after  hour  with  Httle  or  no  sign  of  fatigue.  Fatigue, 
therefore,  in  the  long  run  advantageously  re-dresses  the  balance 
of  an  otherwise  unequal  conflict.  We  can  recognize  in  it  an- 
other agency  working  toward  that  plastic  alternation  of  activi- 
ties which  is  characteristic  of  animal  life  and  increases  in  it  with 
ascent  of  the  animal  scale. 

The  high  variability  of  reflex  reactions  from  experiment  to 
experiment,  and  from  observation  to  observation,  is  admittedly 
one  of  the  difficulties  that  has  retarded  knowledge  of  them. 
Their  variability,  though  often  attributed  to  general  conditions 
of  nutrition,  or  to  local  blood-supply,  etc.,  seems  far  more  often 
due  to  changes  produced  in  the  central  nervous  organ  by  its 
own  functional  conductive  activity  apart  from  fatigue.  This 
functional  activity  itself  causes  from  moment  to  moment  the 
temporary  opening  of  some  connections  and  the  closure  of 
others.  The  chains  of  neurones,  the  conductive  lines,  have 
been,  especially  in  recent  years,  by  the  methods  of  Golgi, 
Ehrlich,  Apathy,  Cajal,  and  others,  richly  revealed  to  the 
microscope.  Anatomical  tracing  of  these  may  be  likened, 
though  more  difficult  to  accomplish,  to  tracing  the  distri- 
bution of  blood-vessels  after  Harvey's  discovery  had  given 
them  meaning,  but  before  the  vasomotor  mechanism  was  dis- 
covered. The  blood-vessels  of  an  organ  may  be  turgid  at  one 
time,  constricted  almost  to  obliteration  at  another.  With  the 
conductive  network  of  the  nervous  system  the  temporal  varia- 
tions are  even  greater,  for  they  extend  to  absolute  withdrawal 
of  nervous  influence.  Under  reflex  inhibition  a  skeletal  muscle 
may  relax  to  its  post-mortem  length,^^  i.  e.y  there  may  then  be  no 
longer  evidence  of  even  a  tonic  influence  on  it  by  its  motor  neu- 
rone.    The  direction  of  the  stream  of  liberation  of  energy  along 


PREPOTENT   REFLEXES  233 

the  pattern  of  the  nervous  web  varies  from  minute  to  minute. 
The  final  common  path  is  handed  from  some  group  of  a  plus 
class  of  afferent  arcs  to  some  group  of  a  minus  class,  or  of  a 
rhythmic  class,  and  then  back  to  one  of  the  previous  groups 
again,  and  so  on.  The  conductive  web  changes  its  func- 
tional pattern  within  certain  limits  to  and  fro.  It  changes  its 
pattern  at  the  entrances  to  common  paths.^^  The  changes  in 
its  pattern  occur  there  in  virtue  of  interaction  between  rival 
reflexes,  "  interference."  As  a  tap  to  a  kaleidoscope,  so  a  new 
stimulus  that  strikes  the  receptive  surface  causes  in  the  central 
organ  a  shift  of  functional  pattern  at  various  synapses.  The 
central  organ  is  a  vast  network  whose  lines  of  conduction  follow 
a  certain  scheme  of  pattern,  but  within  that  pattern  the  details 
Eof  connection  are,  at  the  entrance  to  each  common  path,  muta- 
ble. The  gray  matter  may  be  compared  with  a  telephone  ex- 
change, where,  from  moment  to  moment,  though  the  end-points 
of  the  system  are  fixed,  the  connections  between  starting  points 
and  terminal  points  are  changed  to  suit  passing  requirements, 
as  the  functional  points  are  shifted  at  a  great  railway  junction. 
In  order  to  realize  the  exchange  at  work,  one  must  add  to  its 
purely  spatial  plan  the  temporal  datum  that  within  certain  limits 
the  connections  of  the  lines  shift  to  and  fro  from  minute  to  min- 
ute. An  example  is  the  "  reciprocal  innervation  "  of  antago- 
nistic muscles — when  one  muscle  of  the  antagonistic  couple  is 
thrown  into  action  the  other  is  thrown  out  of  action.  This  is 
only  a  widely  spread  case  of  the  general  rule  that  antagonistic 
reflexes  interfere  where  they  embouch  upon  the  same  final  com- 
mon paths.  And  that  general  rule  is  part  of  the  general  princi- 
ple of  the  mutual  interaction  of  reflexes  that  impinge  upon 
the  same  common  path.  Unlike  reflexes  have  successive  but  not 
simultaneous  use  of  the  common  path  ;  like  reflexes  mutually  re- 
inforce each  other  on  their  common  path.  Expressed  teleologi- 
cally,  the  common  pathy  although  economically  subservient  for  many 
and  various  purposes^  is  adapted  to  serve  but  one  purpose  at  a 
time.  Hence  it  is  a  co-ordinating  mechanism  and  prevents  con- 
fusion by  restricting  the  use  of  the  organ^  its  minister^  to  but  one 
action  at  a  time. 


234  REFLEX   SEQUENCE  [Lect. 

In  the  case  of  simple  antagonistic  muscles,  and  in  the  in- 
stances of  simple  spinal  reflexes,  the  shifts  of  conductive 
pattern  due  to  interaction  at  the  mouths  of  common  paths  are 
of  but  small  extent.  The  co-ordination  covers,  for  instance,  one 
limb  or  a  pair  of  limbs.  But  the  same  principle  extended  to 
the  reaction  of  the  great  arcs  arising  in  the  projicient  receptor 
organs  of  the  head,  e.  g.  the  eye,  which  deal  with  wide  tracts  of 
musculature  as  a  whole^  operates  with  more  multiplex  shift  of 
the  conductive  pattern.  Releasing  forces  acting  on  the  brain 
from  moment  to  moment  shut  out  from  activity  whole  regions 
of  the  nervous  system,  as  they  conversely  call  vast  other  regions 
into  play.  The  resultant  singleness  of  action  from  moment  to 
moment  is  a  key-stone  in  the  constructiojt  of  the  individual  whose 
unity  it  is  the  specific  office  of  the  nervous  system  to  perfect. 
The  interference  of  unlike  reflexes  and  the  alliance  of  like  re- 
flexes in  their  action  upon  their  common  paths  seem  to  lie  at  the 
very  root  of  the  great  psychical  process  of  "  attention." 


VII]       REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS        235 


LECTURE   VII 

REFLEXES   AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS 

Argument :  Reflexes  as  adapted  reactions.  The  purposes  of  various 
type-reflexes.  Shock  a  difficulty  in  deciphering  the  purpose  of  re- 
flexes. Characters  of  spinal  shock.  Its  incidence  confined  to  the 
Iaboral  side  of  the  transection.  Its  difference  in  severity  in  different 
reflexes  and  in  different  animals.  Shock  referable  not  to  the  irrita- 
tion of  the  trauma  but  to  the  cutting  off  by  the  trauma  of  some  supra- 
spinal influence. 
Pseudaffective  reflexes  afford  opportunity  for  determining  the  pain- 
path  in  the  spinal  cord.  This  ascends  both  lateral  columns,  chiefly 
the  one  crossed  from  side  of  stimulation.  The  "  chloroform  cry  " 
in  decerebrate  animals.  Mimesis  of  pleasure  as  compared  with 
mimesis  of  pain.  The  bodily  resonance  of  the  emotions.  The  theory 
of  James,  Lange,  and  Sergi.  Emotional  expressions  in  dogs  deprived 
of  visceral  and  largely  of  bodily  sensation. 

It  is  of  course  as  impossible  to  disprove  as  to  prove  that 
psychical  events  accompany,  or  that  they  do  not  accompany, 
the  nervous  reactions  of  the  **  spinal  "  animal.  It  is  significant, 
however,  that  the  best-known  controversy  (Pfliiger,  Lotze)  as  to 
the  psychical  powers  of  the  spinal  cord,  occurred  prior  to  the 
advent  of  the  Darwinian  theory  of  evolution.  This  latter  sug- 
gests how  purposive  neural  mechanisms  may  arise.  It  furnishes 
a  key  to  the  genesis  and  development  of  adapted  reactions  and, 
among  these  latter,  reflexes. 

That  a  reflex  action  should  exhibit  purpose  is  no  longer 
considered  evidence  that  a  psychical  process  attaches  to  it ;  let 
alone  that  it  represents  any  dictate  of  "  choice  "  or  "  will."  In 
light  of  the  Darwinian  theory  every  reflex  must  be  purposive. 
We  here  trench  upon  a  kind  of  teleology.  It  is  widely  and 
wisely  held  that  natural  knowledge  pursues  the  question  '*  how  " 
rather  than  the  question  "  why."  The  "  why  "  involves  a  judg- 
ment whose  data  lie  so  beyond  present  human  experience  and 
comprehension  that  self-abnegation  in  regard  to  the  desire  to 


236     REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

attempt  it  is  not  only  prudent,  but  to  the  unbiassed  judgment  a 
necessity.  Yet  the  question  has  its  humbler  forms  as  well  as  its 
more  general  and  ambitious. 

Older  writings  on  reflex  action  concerned  themselves  boldly 
with  the  purpose  of  the  reflexes  they  described.  The  language 
in  which  they  are  couched  shows  that  for  them  the  interest  of 
the  phenomena  centred  in  their  being  regarded  as  manifesta- 
tions of  an  informing  spirit  resident  in  the  organism,  lowly 
or  mutilated  though  that  might  be.  Progress  of  knowledge  has 
tended  more  and  more  to  unseat  this  anthropomorphic  image 
of  the  observer  himself  which  he  projected  into  the  object  of 
his  observations.  The  teleological  speculations  accompanying 
such  observations  have  become  proportionately  discredited. 

Self- wounded  in  this  way  physiology  became  for  a  time  ex- 
tremely reticent  about  purpose,  remaining  simply  objectively 
descriptive. 

The  impetus  given  to  biology  by  the  doctrine  of  adaptation 
under  natural  selection,  felt  so  strongly  by  morphological  studies, 
seems  hardly  as  yet  to  have  begun  its  course  as  a  motive  force 
in  physiology.  But  signs  begin  to  be  numerous  that  such  an 
era  is  at  hand.*  The  infinite  fertility  of  the  organism  as  a  field 
for  adapted  reactions  has  become  more  apparent.  The  purpose 
of  a  reflex  seems  as  legitimate  and  urgent  an  object  for  natural 
inquiry  as  the  purpose  of  the  colouring  of  an  insect  or  a  blossom. 
And  the  importance  to  physiology  is,  that  the  reflex  reaction 
cannot  be  really  intelligible  to  the  physiologist  until  he  knows 
its  aim. 

In  general  terms  we  may  say  that  the  effect  of  any  reflex  is 
to  enable  the  organism  in  some  particular  respect  to  better 
dominate  the  environment.  One  often  hears  objection  taken  to 
the  epithets  —  common  in  writings  on  biology  —  "lower"  and 

*  Such  a  "motif"  seems  constantly  present  as  an  undercurrent  in  much 
recent  writing  in  experimental  pathology,  notably  that  of  Ehrlich  in  respect  to  his 
suggestive  "  Antikorper "  hypothesis.  It  is  detectable  as  a  principle  in  the  fine 
researches  by  Bayliss  and  Starling.  Most  definitely  and  broadly  it  is  expressed 
in  the  writings  of  A.  Tschermak,298»  especially  in  the  remarkable  Essay,  Das 
Anpassungs-problem  in  der  Physiologic  der  Gegenwart,  and  in  the  contributions 
of  V.  Uexkiill  from  the  field  of  invertebrate  physiology. 


VII]  "PURPOSE"   IN   REFLEXES  237 

"  higher "  as  applied  to  organisms,  plant  and  animal.  Such 
objection  seems  valid  if  the  phrase  assumes  that  the  "  lower " 
organism  any  less  perfectly  fulfils  its  **  purpose  "  or  "  design  " 
than  does  the  "  higher,"  or  in  those  respects  in  which  it  has 
commerce  with  the  environment  is  any  less  admirably  adjusted 
than  is  the  higher.  But  "lower"  and  "higher"  maybe  used 
without  any  connotation  of  that  kind.  In  the  course  of  evolu- 
tion a  number  of  organisms  have  become  so  adapted  to  the  en- 
vironment as  to  dominate  it  more  variously  and  extensively  than 
do  other  organisms.  In  that  sense  some  organisms  are  higher 
and  some  are  lower.  In  that  sense  man  is  the  highest  organism. 
And  if  evolution  be  a  process  of  gradual  and  more  or  less  un- 
interrupted course  it  is  obvious  that  the  highest  form  achieved 
will  also  be  among  the  latest  of  the  forms  achieved.  This  grad- 
ing of  rank  in  the  animal  scale  will  be  nowhere  more  apparent 
than  in  the  nervous  system  in  its  office  as  integrator  of  the  in- 
dividual. The  more  numerous  and  extensive  the  responses 
made  by  a  creature  to  the  actions  of  the  world  around  upon 
its  receptors,  the  more  completely  will  the  bundle  of  reflexes, 
which  from  this  standpoint  the  creature  is,  figure  the  complexity 
of  the  world  around,  mirroring  it  more  completely  than  do  the 
bundles  of  reflexes  composing  "  lower  "  creatures. 

The  study  of  reflexes  as  adapted  reactions  evidently,  there- 
fore, includes  reactions  of  two  ranks.  With  the  nervous  system 
intact  the  reactions  of  the  various  parts  of  that  system,  the 
"  simple  reflexes,"  are  ever  combined  into  great  unitary  harmo- 
nies, actions  which  in  their  sequence  one  upon  another  consti- 
tute in  their  continuity  what  may  be  termed  the  "  behaviour  " 
(Lloyd  Morgan)  of  the  individual  as  a  whole.  Into  the  intri- 
cate "  purposes  "  (adaptations)  traceable  in  these  total  reactions 
which  constitute  the  creature's  behaviour  as  a  social  unit  in  the 
natural  economy  it  is  not  our  part  to  enter.  Our  part  of  the 
problem  is  a  humbler  one.  In  the  analysis  of  the  animal's  life 
as  a  machine  in  action  there  can  be  split  off  from  its  total 
behaviour  fractional  pieces  which  may  be  treated  conveniently, 
though  artificially,  apart,  and  among  these  are  the  reflexes  we 
have  been  attempting  to  decipher.     We  cannot  but  feel  that 


238     REFLEXES  AS   ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

we  do  not  obtain  due  profit  from  the  study  of  any  particular 
type-reflex  unless  we  can  discuss  its  immediate  purpose  as  an 
adapted  act. 

When  we  try  to  assign  what  we  may  in  this  restricted  sense 
call  its  "  purpose "  to  any  particular  reflex,  the  data  for  an 
answer  are  gathered  in  the  main  from  one  or  other  of  the  con- 
ditions attaching  to  the  reaction.  The  mode  of  the  adequate 
stimulus  is  one  of  these.  The  time-relations  and  spatial  form 
of  the  response  are  others.  The  broad  pressure  applied  under 
the  foot-pad  which  seems  the  adequate  stimulus  ^^  for  the  "  ex- 
tensor-thrust "  and  the  brief  forcible  straightening  of  the  limb 
which  constitutes  the  response  suggest  that  that  reflex  has  its 
purpose  in  the  execution  of  an  act  in  the  series  of  movements 
of  stepping  in  the  animal's  locomotion.  And  the  recent  analysis 
of  Philippson  ^^^  demonstrates  that  such  an  act  occurs  in  the 
reflex  trotting  and  galloping  of  the  dog.  Again,  the  con- 
nection between  the  tickling  irritative  stimuli  which  seem 
"  adequate "  ^^  for  the  "  scratch-reflex,"  and  the  scratching 
movement  itself  which  results,  suggests  that  the  purpose  of  that 
reflex  is  a  grooming  of  the  skin  to  protect  that  organ  against 
parasites  which  infest  it  and  would  confuse  its  function  as  a 
receptive  surface  reacting  to  more  significant  environmental 
stimuli. 

Grainger's  ^^  conclusion  was  that  spinal  cutaneous  reflexes 
"  are  either  of  a  preservative  character  or  resemble  the  move- 
ments which  the  functions  of  the  organ  require."  From  the 
skin  of  the  spinal  creature  reflex  movements  resembling  those 
executed  by  the  normal  individual  in  preening  or  cleansing 
itself  are  of  widespread  occurrence.  Together  with  the  preen- 
ing actions  of  the  spinal  fly,^*  grasshopper,  Astacus,^'®  etc., 
there  fall  into  this  category  the  movement  by  which  the  spinal 
frog  wipes  irritants  from  its  back  or  head ;  the  "  nettoyage " 
by  the  tortoise  ;^^^  the  posturing  of  the  hind  limbs  and  tail  of 
the  spinal  dog  concurrently  with  reflex  defaecation,^^  tending 
to  keep  the  body  from  being  soiled  ;  and  the  **  scratch-reflex  " 
and  the  "  shake-reflex  "  ^^^a  of  the  spinal  dog.  The  conjunctival 
reflex  protecting  the  cornea,  essentially  a  cutaneous  and  from 


VII]  PROTECTIVE   REFLEXES  239 

the  broad  point  of  view  a  spinal  reflex,  is  similarly  preserva- 
tive of  the  part  whence  it  is  initiated. 

There  are  of  course  two  modes  of  preservation,  namely, 
escape  and  defence.  Parts  that  can  move  themselves  seem 
reflexly  to  employ  the  former.  The  spinal  frog's  foot  is  drawn 
out  of  harm's  way  when  irritated ;  so  also  in  the  cat  and  dog. 
But  parts  that  cannot  of  their  own  motion  withdraw  themselves 
effectively  seem  to  invoke  defensive  movements  from  adjacent 
motile  parts.  The  spinal  frog's  flank,  when  irritated,  is  defended 
by  the  hind  limb,  which  comes  up  and  removes  the  irritant  from 
the  flank,  the  flank  itself  also  shrinking  away  somewhat.  Simi- 
larly in  the  scratch-reflex,  the  distant  limb  is  brought  up  to  the 
defence  of  the  irritated  shoulder  or  flank.  There  is  indeed  one, 
rarely  exemplified,  group  of  reflexes  in  which  the  organ  is  sacri- 
ficed for  the  preservation  of  the  rest  of  the  individual.  In  certain 
forms,  e.  g.  Asterias,  Cometula,  Ophiurus,  Arachne,  Carcinus,  a 
limb  pulled  upon  violently  or  long  suddenly  ruptures  itself  and 
is  shed.  These  actions  have  been  shown  by  Fredericq  to  be  re- 
flexes, employing  muscular  contraction.  Such  reactions  exhibit 
well  how  absolutely  the  nervous  system  is  adapted  to  minister 
to  the  requirements  of  the  organism  as  an  integrated  whole,  and 
the  position  of  that  system  as  a  keystone  in  the  upbuilding  of 
the  solidarity  of  the  individual. 

But  the  assignment  of  a  particular  purpose  to  a  particular 
reflex  is  often  difficult  and  hazardous.  The  difficulty  is  inversely 
as  the  amplitude  of  the  field  covered  by  the  reflex-effect.  A 
slight  movement  confined  to  a  single  limb,  or  a  transient  rise  of 
blood-pressure  observed  alone,  is  open  to  many  interpretations 
and  admits  of  no  security  of  inference.  It  is  a  fractional  reaction 
that  may  belong  to  any  of  many  general  reactions  of  varied  aim. 

When  a  reflex  is  elicited  faintly  in  a  spinal  animal  it  occurs 
simply  at  the  focus,  so  to  say,  of  its  area  of  distribution,  and 
owing  to  the  restricted  character  of  its  features  its  meaning  may 
be  difficult  or  impossible  to  read.  It  is  in  my  experience  only 
by  repeated  observations  of  a  reflex  under  various  circumstances 
of  its  development  that  as  a  rule  its  significance  becomes  clear. 
The  accessory  parts  of  it  are  often  instructive  concerning  the 


240    REFLEXES  AS   ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

whole.  In  the  scratch-reflex  of  the  dog,  besides  the  rhythmic 
scratching  movement  of  the  hind  hmb,  say  of  the  right,  there  is 
steady  extension  of  the  left  hind  limb,  and  steady  extension  with 
some  abduction  of  the  two  fore  limbs.  The  accessory  parts 
of  the  reflex,  namely  those  in  the  three  limbs  which  are  not 
scratching,  are  also  contributory  to  the  same  effect  as  in  the 
scratching  movement  in  the  right  hind  limb  itself  They  steady 
the  dog  and  secure  the  stabiHty  of  its  body  during  the  perform- 
ance of  the  scalptor  act. 

In  the  "  flexion-reflex  "  of  the  hind  limb  excited  by  noxious 
stimuli,  e.  g.  a  prick  or  a  faradic  current,  the  limb  itself  is  drawn 
up, — if  weakly,  chiefly  by  flexion  at  the  knee  ;  if  strongly,  by  flex- 
ion at  hip  as  strongly  as  at  knee.  At  the  same  time  the  crossed 
hind  limb  is  thrown  into  action,  primarily  in  extension,  but  this 
is  soon  followed  by  flexion,  and  alternating  extension  and  flexion 
is  the  characteristic  result.  The  rate  of  this  alternation  is  about 
twice  a  second.  That  is  to  say,  the  foot  which  has  stamped  on 
the  thorn  is  drawn  up  out  of  way  of  further  wounding,  and  the 
fellow  hind  limb  runs  away;  and  so  do  the  fore  legs  when  — 
which  is  more  difficult  to  arrange,  owing  to  the  height  of  the 
necessary  spinal  transection  —  they  also  are  included,  fairly  free 
from  shock,  within  the  "  spinal "  animal. 

Spinal  shock.  One  of  the  experimental  difficulties  in  de- 
ciphering the  purport  of  a  spinal  reflex  is  the  phenomenon 
known  as  "  shock."  "  If  in  a  frog  the  spinal  marrow  be  divided 
just  behind  the  occiput,  there  are  for  a  very  short  time  no  dias- 
taltic  actions  in  the  extremities.  The  diastaltic  actions  speedily 
return.     This  phenomenon  is  *  shock.'  "      (Marshall  Hall.)^ 

Whytt  had,  a  century  previous  to  Hall,  drawn  attention 
to  the  same  phenomenon,  although  assigning  to  it  no  descriptive 
term.  The  whole  of  that  depression  or  suppression  of  nervous 
functions  which  ensues  forthwith  upon  a  mechanical  injury  of 
some  part  of  the  nervous  system  and  is  of  temporary  nature 
may  be  conveniently  included  as  "  shock."  Goltz  considered  it 
entirely  a  collection  of  inhibition  phenomena.  Among  labora- 
tory animals  it  is  in  the  monkey  that,  on  the  whole,  "  spinal 
shock  "  appears  at  maximum. 


ii 


I 


SPINAL  SHOCK  241 


Spinal  shock  appears  to  take  effect  in  the  aboral  direction 
Qj^ly  183,  205  Section  behind  the  brachial  enlargement  disturbs  little 
if  at  all  the  reactions  of  the  fore  limb,  although  the  number 
of  headward  running  channels  of  conduction  ruptured  by  such  a 
section  is  enormous.  Striking  instances  of  the  absence  of  head- 
ward  spread  of  the  depression  due  to  "  shock  "  are  afforded  by 
transections  abutting  on  the  lower  edge  of  the  fifth  cervical  seg- 
ment; these  depress  the  respiratory  activity  of  the  phrenic 
motor  cells  hardly  at  all,  even  momentarily.  On  the  aboral 
side  of  the  transection  depression  is  profound.  Analogously, 
the  sudden  cutting  off  of  all  that  stream  of  centripetal  impulses 
continually  pouring  for  conscious  and  subconscious  elaboration 
into  the  encephalon  from  the  cutaneous,  articular,  and  muscular 
senscHDrgans  of  the  tail,  limbs,  trunk,  and  neck,  and  from  the 
viscera,  seems  to  disturb  the  reactions  of  the  head  and  brain 
little  or  not  at  all. 

After  high  cervical  transection,  *'  shock "  appears  more 
severe  in  the  fore  Hmbs  than  in  the  hind.  For  an  hour  or  so  it 
may  be  difficult  to  elicit  any  reflex  movement  from  skin  inner- 
vated behind  the  transection,  whether  by  mechanical,  thermal, 
or  electrical  stimuli. 

The  view  of  Goltz  and  his  school  that  **  spinal  shock "  is 
a  long  lasting  inhibition  due  to  irritation  by  trauma  is  not, 
I  think,  really  tenable.  The  argument  implies,  if  it  does 
not  explicitly  state,  that  the  trauma,  by  its  damage  and  by  its 
subsequent  processes  of  inflammatory  reaction,  formation  of 
scar-tissue,  etc.,  acts  as  a  stimulus,  exciting  inhibition  that  de- 
presses or  suppresses  reflex  activity  in  adjacent  and  even  remote 
arcs  of  the  central  nervous  system.  Against  this  explanation 
militate  several  facts.  Firstly,  the  shock  takes  effect  almost 
exclusively  in  the  aboral  direction.  Were  the  mere  irritative 
action  of  the  trauma  the  cause,  it  is  not  easy  to  see  why  the 
nervous  centres  near  the  trauma  should  not  be  depressed  on 
either  side  of,  for  instance,  a  spinal  transection,  headward  as  well 
as  backward.  Secondly,  experiments  of  the  following  kind  give 
results  difficult  to  reconcile  with  the  view.  When  in  the  dog 
complete  transection  of  the  spinal  cord  through  the  eighth  cer- 

16 


242     REFLEXES  AS   ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

vical  segment  is  practised,  a  severe  fall  in  the  general  arterial 
blood-pressure  ensues,  and  vasomotor  reflexes  cannot  be  elic- 
ited. But  in  the  course  of  some  days  this  is  largely  recovered 
from,  and  after  some  weeks  the  blood-pressure  will,  with  the 
animal  in  the  horizontal  position,  often  be  found  practically 
normal.  When  the  animal  is  then  anaesthetized  and  curarized, 
artificial  respiration  being  maintained,  it  is  usually  easy  to  obtain 
on  stimulation  of  the  central  ends  of  divided  afferent  or  mixed 
nerves,  for  instance  of  the  internal  saphenous  nerve,  good  and 
often  very  large  vasomotor  reflexes,  the  blood- pressure  rising 
fifty  millimeters  and  more  (Fig.  6^),  These  reflexes  upon  the 
vascular  musculature  are  purely  spinal,  since  the  cord  has  been 
divided  just  headward  of  the  thoracic  region.  Then,  while  these 
spinal  vasomotor  reflexes  are  regularly  elicitable  and  serve  as 
a  guide  to  the  reflex  activity  of  the  cord  behind  the  transection, 
I  have  transected  the  cord  again  a  couple  of  segments  behind  the 
original  transection.  This  section  excites  an  immediate  transient 
rise  in  the  arterial  pressure,  lasting  about  a  minute,  and  succeeded 
by  a  gradual  fall.  The  arterial  pressure,  then,  in  my  experience 
sinks  to  an  equilibrium  of  pressure  hardly  lower  than  its  mean 
prior  to  this  second  transection.     There  is  none  of  that  deep 


200  mm.  Hg. 

^^^■■■1 

150  mm.  Hg. 

^l^^^^l 

100  mm.  Hg- 
b.  p. 

^^^^^^B 

Signal 

^^^^^^^^^H 

Time  in  2" 

^^BBBBJii^B^^^^M 

Figure  67.  —  Spinal  vasomotor  reflex;  dog;  300  days  after  spinal  transection  at  8th  cervical 
level;  chloroform  and  curare.  Electrical  stimulation  of  central  end  of  a  digital  nerve  of 
hind  limb  during  the  time  marked  by  signal  on  second  line  from  bottom.  The  arterial 
pressure  (carotid)  rises  from  90  mm.  Hg  to  208  mm.  Hg.   Time  marked  below  in  2  seconds. 


VII]  SPINAL   SHOCK  243 

depression  which  ensued  on  the  first  trauma,  though  the  second 
trauma  has  been  practically  qua  trauma  a  complete  repetition  of 
the  former  one.  If  the  fall  of  general  blood-pressure  be  regarded 
as  part,  and  a  severe  part,  of  the  "  spinal "  shock  which  ensues 
on  spinal  transection  in  the  cervical  region,  the  absence  of  that 
fall  on  repeating  practically  the  same  trauma  must  signify  that 
the  second  trauma  is  not  followed  by  the  shock  that  followed  the 
first  trauma.  Moreover,  reflex  heightenings  of  blood-pressure 
such  as  were  regularly  obtainable  just  prior  to  the  second  tran- 
section are  obtainable  immediately,  /.  e.  four  minutes,  after  the 
second  spinal  transection.  The  first  trauma  causes  temporary  1 
deep  depression  of  the  spinal  tonus  of  the  vascular  system  and  ; 
temporary  abolition  of  vascular  reflexes.  The  second  trauma  \ 
causes  practically  no  depression,  even  transient,  of  the  re-estab- 
lished tonus  of  the  vascular  system  nor  of  the  pressor  spinal  vas- 
cular reflexes  that  have  become  similarly  re-established.  It  may 
perhaps  be  objected  that  the  vascular  tonus  established  subse- 
quent to  the  first  spinal  transection  is  of  peripheral  mechanism 
and  outside  the  spinal  cord  itself.  That  that  is  not  its  main 
factor  is  shown  by  the  further  deep  depression  of  vascular  tonus 
which  occurs  when  the  spinal  cord  in  the  thoracic  region  is 
itself  not  merely  transected  but  destroyed. 

The  trauma  qua  trauma  is  as  severe  in  the  first  instance  as 
in  the  second.     In  these  experiments,  therefore,  the  "  shock  "  is 
not  due  to  the  trauma  qua  trauma.     It  seems  to  depend  simply 
on  solution  of  continuity  of  nervous  channels,  and  this  solution 
is  practically  equally  great  whether  the  actual  trauma  itself  be 
relatively  slight  (a  clean,  sharply  cut  transection)  or  relatively 
severe  (a  contused  and  jagged  transrupture),  so  long  as  in  the 
two  cases  it  involves  an  equal  amount  of  the  transverse  area  of; 
the  cord.     The  practical  absence  of  spinal  **  shock  "  on  repeti-  \ 
tion  of  the  trauma  further  back  is  explicable  by  its  then  causing  1 
little   further   aggravation    of  the  interruption  of  the  nervous 
channels  concerned   with  vascular  tone  and  vascular   reflexes,  j 
those  channels  having  already  been  ruptured  by  the  previous 
transection  somewhat  further  headward. 

Similarly  the  flexion-reflex  of  the  hind  limb,  though  it  suffers 


244     REFLEXES   AS   ADAPTED    REACTIONS       [Lect. 

considerably  from  shock  after  transection  of  the  cord  in  the 
hinder  cervical  or  thoracic  region,  when  it  has  recovered  is  but 
little,  and  but  briefly,  depressed  by  a  second  transection  made 
behind  the  previous.  In  this  case  also  trauma  does  not,  there- 
fore, account  for  the  spinal  shock.  The  shock  following  the 
trauma  is  proportioned  not  to  the  mere  wound,  but  to  the  num- 
ber and  character  of  the  descending  nerve-paths  through  which 
the  lesion  breaks.  Porter's  ^^^  *  well-known  experiment  on  the 
respiratory  intraspinal  path  from  the  bulb  to  the  phrenic  neu- 
rones points  to  the  same  conclusion. 

There  remains  the  further  question  as  to  whether  spinal 
"  shock "  is  a  phenomenon  of  inhibition.  A  reflex  during  its 
depression  by  spinal  shock  does  not  present  the  features  it 
shows  when  reduced  by  inhibition  so  much  as  features  resem- 


FiGURE  68.  —  Scratch-reflex  under  "  spinal  shock."  Spinal  transection  six  weeks  previously. 
The  reflex  was  elicited  by  vigorous  mechanical  stimulation,  electrical  stimulation  being,  as 
is  usual  under  shock,  unable  to  evoke  it.  The  time  of  application  of  the  stimulus  is  marked 
by  the  signal  line,  which  also  records  the  time  in  fifths  of  seconds.  The  reflex  is  slow  to 
appear,  feeble  and  irregular,  and  lapses  during  the  continuance  of  the  stimulation.  The 
small  waves  on  the  base  line  are  due  to  the  vigorous  rubbing  necessary  to  evoke  the  reflex 
at  all,  and  are  simply  mechanically  conveyed  to  the  limb  attached  to  the  myograph. 


i! 


VII]  SPINAL  SHOCK  245 


Figure  69.  —  Spinal  shock.  The  scratch-reflex,  as  in  the  previous  figure,  but  obtained  under 
still  deeper  depression  of  spinal  shock.  Time  in  fifths  of  seconds.  The  signal  shows  the 
period  of  mechanical  excitation. 

bling  those  characteristic  of  it  when  fatigued.  The  scratch- 
reflex  under  spinal  shock  (Figs.  6S,  69)  shows  irregularity  of 
rhythm,  slow  protracted  relatively  feeble  beats,  and  speedy 
onset  of  temporary  inexcitability,  features  which  characterize  it 
when  nearly  tired  out  (comp.  Fig.  62,  63,  Lect.  VI).  So  also 
with  the  flexion-reflex  of  the  leg.  In  the  period  of  depression 
by  spinal  shock  the  reflex  is  feeble  even  under  strong  excitation, 
is  relatively  short-lasting,  and  on  cessation  of  the  exciting  stim- 
ulus shows  little  of  the  prolonged  after-discharge  that  it  is 
prone  to  show  at  other  times :  it  also  tires  out  then  with 
abnormal  rapidity.  The  scratch-reflex  in  spinal  shock  of 
pronounced  degree  fails  to  be  elicitable  by  electrical  stim- 
ulation at  all,  though  still  elicitable  by  rubbing.  This  indi- 
cates the  greater  efficacy  of  a  stimulus  more  nearly  like  the 
adequate. 

The  condition  of  the  spinal  reflex-arcs  in  spinal  shock  ap- 
pears to  resemble  a  general  spinal  fatigue  rather  than  an  inhibi- 
tion.    It  renders  difficult  and  uncertain  the  process  of  conduction 


246     REFLEXES   AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

along  the  reflex-arc  as  judged  by  the  discharge  from  the  ter- 
minal neurone.  This  suggests  a  loosening  of  nexus  between 
the  links  of  the  neurone-chain  composing  the  arc ;  a  defect  of 
transmission  at  the  synapse.  Such  a  conception  of  the  disorder 
accords  well  with  the  suggestion  of  v.  Monakow^^  that  a  ''  dia- 
schizis  "  takes  place  between  the  conducting  cells,  his  "  schalt- 
zellen  "  failing  to  perform  their  normal  function  as  connecting 
elements. 

I  think,  therefore,  that  spinal  shock  is  neither  due  to  irritation 
by  trauma,  nor  in  the  main  a  phenomenon  of  inhibition.  The 
rupture  of  certain  aborally  conducting  paths  appears  to  induce 
it.  Which  these  paths  exactly  are  is  matter  for  research.  After 
cervical  transection  separating  the  cord  from  the  bulbar  vaso- 
motor centre,  the  phenomenon  might  be  attributable  to  the 
invariably  severe  fall  of  general  arterial  pressure.  But  this 
cannot  be  the  chief  explanation,  for:  (i)the  head  does  not  par- 
ticipate in  the  "  shock,"  although  participating  in  the  low  blood- 
pressure;  (2)  with  post-thoracic  transection  the  body  region 
distal  to  the  spinal  lesion  exhibits  shock  as  severe  as  after  cervical 
transection,  though  there  is  no  fall  of  blood-pressure ;  (3)  tran- 
section anterior  to  the  bulbar  vasomotor  centre  but  posterior  to 
the  pons  leaves  the  blood-pressure  unreduced  but  the  spinal 
shock  severe. 

The  shock  is  more  profound  in  the  monkey  than  in  other  ani- 
mals observed  in  the  laboratory.  This  might  suggest  a  cerebral 
origin  for  the  paths  implicated.  But  ablation  of  the  hemispheres 
does  not  induce  anything  like  the  depth  of  spinal  depression  in- 
duced by  transections  behind  the  pons.  The  much  severer 
character  of  the  depression  when  the  transection  passes  behind 
the  pons  indicates  an  aborally  directed  influence  from  some 
nucleus  of  the  pontine  or  midbrain  system,  driven  probably 
by  the  great  cranial  receptors  of  otic  labyrinth  and  eye,  rein- 
forced by  impulses  from  the  cord  itself.  The  great  influence 
on  spinal  centres  of  a  cranial  mechanism  in  this  region,  driven 
by  the  otic  labyrinth,  is  illustrated  by  Ewald's  ^^  proposed  name 
"  tonus  labyrinth  "  for  the  end-organ  of  the  octavus  nerve.  The 
great  severity  of  shock  in  the  monkey  would  accord  with  high 


1^ 


IIP 


SPINAL   SHOCK  247 


exercise  of  function  of  this  apparatus  in  an  animal  endowed  with 
such  variety  and  range  of  skeletal  movement. 

In  the  monkey  and  in  man  spinal  shock  is  not  only  pecul- 
iarly intense  but  peculiarly  long  lasting.  The  withdrawal  from 
the  isolated  cord  of  influences  it  is  wont  to  receive  from 
centres  further  headward  may  induce  an  alteration  of  trophic 
character  in  spinal  cells  —  an  *'  isolation  dystrophy  "  ^^  —  visible, 
it  may  be,  as  Nissl's  chromatolysis.  This  "  isolation-dystrophy  " 
ensuing  on  shock  would  add  itself  as  a  longer  lasting,  in  some 
elements  perhaps  a  permanent,  depression.  Certainly  spinal 
transection  is  followed  in  the  monkey  by  longer  lasting  "  shock  " 
—  included  in  which  I  suspect  is  "isolation-dystrophy"  —  than 
in  other  animal  types  observed  in  the  laboratory.  My  results 
in  monkeys  bore  out  that  which  Bastian,^^  Bowlby,^^^  and 
Bruns,^*^  contrary  to  previous  observers,  have  described  as  the 
typical  condition  in  man  after  spinal  injury  completely  severing 
the  cord.  Thus,  I  found  the  knee-jerk  sometimes  inelicitable 
during  a  month  or  so  after  midthoracic  transection  in  the  monkey, 
whereas  in  the  rabbit  its  abeyance  lasts  usually  but  ten  minutes 
or  a  quarter  of  an  hour. 

It  is  noteworthy  that  spinal  shock  takes  effect  on  just 
those  tissues  which  waste  when  the  synaptic  nervous  system  is 
destroyed  —  namely  the  skeletal  muscles.  Where  the  primitive 
diffuse  nervous  system,  the  nerve-net,  exists,  as  in  the  visceral  and 
vascular  musculature,  neither  **  spinal  shock  "  nor  atrophy  occur 
consequently  to  spinal  transection.  In  the  skeletal  muscles  the 
"  spindles "  do  not  waste.^^  Jamin  found  that  the  disuse- 
wasting  of  the  muscles  in  spinal  dogs  which  had  been  daily 
exercised  in  reflex  actions  in  my  laboratory  was  much  less  than 
in  other  dogs  he  examined.  The  organs  on  which  "  shock " 
falls  least  heavily  are  those  which  suffer  least  even  after  exsec- 
tion  of  the  spinal  cord  itself 

The  deeper  depression  of  reaction  into  which  the  higher  ani- 
mal as  contrasted  with  the  lower  sinks  when  made  "  spinal," 
appears  to  me  ^^  significant  of  this,  that  in  the  higher  types, 
more  than  in  the  lower,  the  great  cerebral  senses  actuate  the 
motor  organs  and  impel  the  motions  of  the  individual. 


248     REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

"  Spinal  shock "  does  not  fall  upon  all  reflexes  with  equal 
severity.  Noci-ceptive  reflexes  suffer  relatively  slightly.  In 
the  dog,  after  spinal  transection  in  the  posterior  part  of  the 

cervical  region,  the  reflexes  acting  on  the  muscles  of  the  hind  | 
limb  show  less  severe  and  shorter-lasting  depression  in  regard 

to  the  flexion-reflex   and  in  regard  to  the  scratch-reflex  than  | 

to  the  extensor-thrust.     That   may  explain  why  a  number  of  I 

observers  have    not  obtained   any  homonymous  reflex  of  ex-  | 

tension  in  the  spinal  mammal.     The  crossed  extension-reflex,  1 

which  is  really  a  part  of  the  great  reflex  of  which  homony-  | 

mous  flexion  is  the    more    prominent   feature,   recovers   from  f 

spinal  shock  earlier  than  does  the  extensor-thrust  ^ 

There  is  variability  in  the  order  of  recovery  of  the  various  | 
spinal  reflexes  of  the  dog  from  spinal  shock.  Occasionally  i 
the  scratch-reflex  returns  as  early  as  the  flexion-reflex.  Al-  i 
though  usually  in  the  hind  limbs  of  the  spinal  dog  no  extensor  | 
rigidity  develops,  in  some  individuals  it  does  so.  The  limbs  | 
are  kept  extended  at  knee  and  ankle  even  to  a  degree  that  it  | 
is  difficult  to  break  through  by  the  inhibition  accompanying  i 
elicitation  of  the  flexion-reflex  on  stimulation  of  the  foot.  ; 
It  is  not  difficult  to  see  how  this  may  come  about.  Some  i 
incidental  circumstance  determining  the  preponderance  of  some  ; 
passive  attitude  of  the  limbs  during  the  early  days  succeeding  ] 
the  lesion  may,  by  its  influence  on  the  interaction  of  the  re-  i 
covering  spinal  arcs,  impress  an  unwonted  reflex  habit  upon 
the  limbs.  It  is  not  uncommon  to  find,  especially  in  the  spinal  i 
monkey,  I  think,  differences  in  the  reflex  condition  of  the  right  i 
and  left  limbs,  even  although  the  spinal  transection  has  given 
a  perfectly  symmetrical  spinal  lesion.  Such  inequality  or  dis-  [ 
similarity  of  the  spinal  reflexes  right  and  left  does  not  neces-  '■ 
sarily  afford  any  evidence  that  the  spinal  lesion  is  asymmetrical.  1 
Intercurrent  circumstances  suffice  to  impress  slightly  different  ' 
reflex  habits  on  the  two  limbs,  and  in  one  and  the  same  in- 
dividual the  reflex  habits  of  each  limb  may  vary  somewhat  from  ; 
period  to  period  (^/  Lewandowsky  on  the  production  of  . : 
hemiplegic  contracture).  fi 

Local  sign  in  reflexes.     The  /oais  of  the  stimulus  plays  an  ; 


VII]  LOCAL   SIGN   IN   REFLEXES  249 

important  part  in  determining  the  nature  of  the  reflex  evoked. 
This  influence  of  the  location  of  the  stimulus  on  the  resulting 
reflex  movement  has  been  one  of  the  features  most  studied 
in  reflex  action.  It  furnishes  a  large  part  of  the  direct  evidence 
of  the  "purposive"  character  of  spinal  reflexes.^ 

The  rule  of  spatial  proximity  given  above  partly  expresses 
the  influence  of  this  factor.  Much  that  was  mentioned  regarding 
long  irradiation  illustrates  it  further.  Though  the  importance 
of  the  locus  is  high  when  broadly  taken,  it  does  not  appear  obvi- 
ous as  attaching  to  small  differences  of  location  in  a  more  or 
less  homogeneous  receptive  field  or  area.  Yet  in  such  a  field 
the  reflexes,  though  similar,  are  demonstrably  not  identical.^^^ 
In  the  spinal  monkey,  excitation  of  the  outer  edge  of  \hQ  platita 
while  causing  dorso-flexion  at  ankle,  in  doing  so  generally 
brings  the  peronei  into  play  more  than  is  the  case  when  the 
flexion  is  excited  from  the  inner  edge  of  the  planta ;  then  tibialis 
anticus  predominates,  causing  some  inversion.  In  the  frog, 
excitation  of  the  skin  of  the  dorsal  aspect  of  the  knee,  and  of 
the  ventral  aspect  respectively,  alike  evoke  flexion  at  hip,  knee, 
and  ankle,  but  in  the  former  case  the  foot  is  somewhat  everted, 
in  the  latter  somewhat  inverted.  We  must  allow  that  the  cen- 
tripetal impulses,  although  they  yield  no  sensation,  yet  possess,  to 
borrow  a  term  from  the  psychologist,  "  local  sign."  In  the  naked 
eye  Medusa,  called  on  account  of  its  localizing  reflexes  Tiaropsis 
indicans^*'^  the  manubrium  deflects  itself  towards  the  stimulated 
part  of  the  nectocalyx;  its  tip  is  brought  with  precision  to 
meet  the  concurrently  contracted  inbent  portion  of  the  necto- 
calyx. If  one  point  of  the  nectocalyx  be  irritated,  and  while  the 
manubrium  is  applied  to  that  point,  then  another,  the  manubrium 
will  leave  the  first  point  and  move  over  to  the  second.  In  this 
way  it  may  be  made  to  indicate  successively  a  number  of  points 
of  irritation.  "  After  a  series  of  such  irritations  the  manubrium 
subsequently  continues  for  some  time  to  visit  first  one  and  then 

1  The  "rule  of  spatial  proximity"  offers  an  explanation  for  many  of  those 
minor  differences  obtaining  in  broadly  similar  reflex  movements,  there  being  a 
tendency  for  the  muscles  belonging  to  the  immediate  spinal  vicinity  of  the  skin 
stimulated  to  respond  in  preponderant  degree  ;  similarly  in  the  scratch-reflex. 


250    REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

another  of  the  points  which  have  been  irritated."  ^^  A  cut  be- 
tween the  base  of  the  manubrium  and  the  point  of  irritation  in 
the  bell  destroys  the  localization,  though  movement  occurs 
toward  some  part  of  the  quadrant  of  the  bell  containing  the  site 
of  stimulus;  but  the  accuracy  of  the  localization  is  reduced. 
The  reaction  recalls  the  bending  of  the  tentacles  of  Drosera^ 
in  the  direction  needful  to  reach  the  seat  of  stimulation  on  the 
leaf.  The  headless  bee  stings  in  response  to  stimulation  of  the 
under-surface  pretty  accurately  at  the  site  of  irritation.^'^  In 
the  "spinal"  crayfish,  if  one  leg  is  caught  it  is  flexed  and  drawn 
up,  and  soon  all  the  others,  if  the  leg  is  not  released,  are 
brought  round  it  and  push  at  the  hand  holding  the  limb."^*  The 
yellow  clover-fly  will,  after  decapitation,  stand  cleaning  its  wings 
with  its  hind  legs,  and  clean  its  "three  pairs  of  legs,  rubbing 
them  together  in  a  determined  manner,  and  raising  its  fore  legs 
vainly  in  air  as  if  searching  for  its  head  to  brush  up."^  But 
in  Astacus  the  accuracy  of  localization  is  much  impaired  on 
the  crossed  side  by  cutting  the  cross  commissures  combining 
the  ganglia  most  closely  concerned  with  the  reaction.^^  This 
recalls  the  effect  of  the  tangential  cut  in  the  nectocalyx  of 
Tiaropsis?'^ 

In  a  reflex  reaction  exhibiting  "  local  sign  "  in  the  above  sense, 
the  afferent  impulses  involved  are  divisible  into  several  groups 
according  to  their  place  of  origin.  There  must  be  (i)  a  group 
originated  at  the  seat  of  stimulus,  (2)  a  group  initiated  in  the 
motor  and  mobile  organs  reflexly  set  in  action,  and  (3)  in 
some  cases  a  group  arising  at  the  distant  spot  to  which  the 
movement  is  directed.  Regarding  this  last  group  an  experi- 
ment illustrates  its  extinction  without  extinction  of  the  "  local 
sign."  Thus,  in  Astacus}'^  after  section  of  the  nerve-cords 
behind  the  mouth,  when,  therefore,  the  hind  creature  without 
mouth  has  lost  all  nervous  connection  with  the  front  creature 
possessing  the  mouth,  food  given  to  the  claws  of  the  hind 
creature  is  still  at  once  and  accurately  carried  by  them  to  the 
mouth,  and  this  latter  may  refuse  to  take  the  morsel  brought. 
In  the  grasshopper,^^^a  after  extirpation  of  the  supra  and 
suboesophageal  ganglia  (entire   brain),  the  front   leg   is   pro- 


PSEUDAFFECTIVE   REFLEXES  251 

tracted,  and  in  the  normal  way  catches  the  antenna,  and  the 
usual  movements  of  cleaning  the  antenna  go  on,  although  the 
antenna  has  entirely  lost  its  innervation  owing  to  the  destruc- 
tion of  the  brain.  Regarding  the  second  mentioned  group  of 
afferent  impulses,  H.  E.  Hering^^^  has  made  the  interesting 
observation  that  the  "cleansing"  reflex  of  the  spinal  frog  which 
brings  the  foot  to  a  seat  of  irritation  on  the  dorsal  or  perineal 
skin  is  accurately  executed  after  severance  of  the  afferent  spinal 
roots  of  the  limb  itself.  In  the  same  way  the  bulbo-spinal  frog 
brings  the  fore  limb  to  the  snout  when  the  snout  is  stimulated 
after  section  of  the  afferent  roots  of  the  fore  limb.  The  scratcli-| 
reflex  I  find  executed  without  obvious  impairment  of  direction! 
or  rhythm  when  all  the  afferent  roots  of  the  scratching  hind  limbj 
have  been  cut  through.  In  the  execution  of  these  spinal  reflexes, 
therefore,  the  most  important  afferent  factor  as  regards  "  local 
sign  "  is  the  afferent  channel  from  the  place  of  initiation  of  the 
reflex. 

Pseudaffective  reflexes.  If  we  turn  to  reflex-effects  excited 
by  nocuous  stimulation  of  the  skin  but  having  for  their  field 
of  development  a  wider  conjunction  of  reflex-arcs  and  con- 
sequently a  wider  mechanism  of  reflex  expression,  the  reflex 
response  seems  to  indicate  yet  more  clearly  the  "  purpose  "  of 
the  reflex. 

If  from  the  cat  under  deep  chloroform  narcosis  the  cerebral 
hemispheres  and  part  of  the  thalami  be  removed,  on  relaxing 
the  narcosis  a  number  of  motor  reactions  can  be  observed 
against  the  background  of  '^decerebrate  rigidity'' ^^  Among 
these  reactions  are  some  mimetic  movements  simulating  ex- 
pression of  certain  affective  states.  These  "pseudaffective" 
reflexes  Woodworth  and  myself ^^  have  endeavoured  to  use  for 
elucidation  of  the  spinal  path  conducting  those  impulses  that, 
were  the  brain  intact,  would,  we  may  presume,  evoke  "  pain." 
The  search  for  such  a  path  is,  as  regards  channels  from  skin,  a 
search  for  a  path  as  specific  as  those  of  the  special  senses. 
The  truncation  of  the  brain  of  the  mammal  at  the  mesenceph- 
alon annihilates  the  neural  mechanism  to  which  the  affective 
psychosis  is  adjunct.    But  it  leaves  fairly  intact  the  reflex  motor 


252     REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect.     \ 

machinery  whose  concurrent  action  is  habitually  taken  as  out- 
ward expression  of  an  inward  feeling.     When  the  expression 
occurs  it  may  be  assumed  that,  had  the  brain  been  present,  the 
feeling  would  have  occurred.     Pain  is  the  psychical  adjunct  of 
a  protective  reflex.     A  spinal  translesion  which  prevents  occur- 
rence of  the  expression  in  response  to  a  stimulus  that  previously 
excited  the  expression  has  therefore  been  regarded  by  us  in  the 
following  experiments  to   be  such  as  would,  were   the   brain 
present,  induce    analgesia  in  regard   to   that   stimulus.      Even 
apart  from  that  assumption,  it  is  clear  that  such  a  lesion  can  be 
used   for  determining   the    conducting   path  of  a   noci-ceptive     i 
reaction.     The  spinal  path  concerned  with  the  forward  trans-     ' 
mission    of  these    impulses   can    therefore    be   designated   not 
merely  a  headward  path,  but,  having  regard  to  the  character  of 
the  reaction,  the  headward  path  for  noci-ceptive  (p.  266)  reactions. 
The  reflex-effect  observed  has  presented  the  following  ele- 
ments :  diagonal  cyclic  movements  of  the  limbs  as  in  progres- 
sion (sometimes  producing  progression),  turning  of  head  and     j 
neck    toward    the   point   stimulated;    opening   of  the    mouth,     I 
retraction  of  the  lips  and  tongue,  movement  of  the  vibrissae ; 
snapping  of  the  jaw ;  lowering  of  the  head  ;  opening  of  the  eye- 
lids, dilatation  of  the  pupils;  vocalization  angry  in  tone  (snarl- 
ing), sometimes  plaintive;  and  with  these  a  transient  increase 
of  arterial  blood-pressure.     These  reactions  appear  not  only  in     \ 
combination,  but  sometimes  singly  or  in  small  combinations. 
The    most  readily  elicitable  are  movements  of  the  vibrissae, 
opening  of  the  mouth  with  retraction  of  the  tongue,  and  lower- 
ing of  the  head ;   but  though  in  some  cases  vigorous  and  prompt 
they  never  amount  to  an  effective  action  of  attack  or  escape.     • 
A  characteristic  feature  of  their  ineffectiveness  is  their   brief    j 
duration.   -The  movement,  even  when  most  vigorous  and  prompt,     I 
dies  away  rapidly,  to   be  succeeded   in  some  cases  by  a  few     | 
weaker  repetitions,  each  in  succession  weaker  and  more  tran- 
sient than  the  last.     Thus,  the  movements  of  the  head   may 
recur   three   or  four   times  in  response   to  a  single   stimulus, 
or  the  vocalization  be  repeated  in  a  diminishing  series  for  a 

minute  or  so.  "  \ 

1 


VII]  SPINAL   PATH   FOR  PAIN  253 

Our  method  has  been  to  compare  by  means  of  the  above 
reaction  the  effect  of  two  stimuli  symmetrically  but  succes- 
sively applied  on  opposite  sides  of  the  body,  after  a  semisection 
or  other  lesion  of  the  spinal  cord  headward  of  the  entrance  of 
the  nervepath  stimulated. 

After  semisection  at  the  13th  thoracic  level  the  pseud- 
affective  reaction  was  obtained  by  stimulation  bf  either  sciatic 
trunk,  but  more  vigorously  and  promptly  from  the  nerve  of  the 
side  of  the  semisection :  from  this  nerve  also  the  reaction  was 
evoked  by  weaker  faradization.  This  indicates  that  the  head- 
ward  pathway  taken  by  the  impulses  eliciting  the  vocal  and 
other  pseudaffective  reactions  is  from  the  hind  limb  both  crossed 
and  uncrossed,  but  is  more  largely  crossed.  From  our  experi- 
ments we  are  able  to  exclude  the  dorsal  spinal  column  as  the 
main  path  of  conduction.  Sections  of  both  dorsal  columns  made 
no  appreciable  difference  in  the  reaction  to  the  stimulus;  neither 
did  faradization  of  them  evoke  the  reaction.  The  median  por- 
tion of  the  ventral  column  has  sometimes  been  trespassed  on 
in  making  the  semisection  of  the  opposite  side ;  this  extension 
of  the  lesion  has  not  prevented  the  reaction  from  occurring. 
In  one  case  the  whole  gray  matter  of  both  halves  of  the  cord 
was  found  at  the  autopsy  to  be  heavily  infiltrated  and  ploughed 
up  with  extravasated  blood  at  the  level  of  the  semisection  and 
for  several  millimetres  both  ahead  and  behind  it.  It  must  have 
been  largely,  if  not  completely,  thrown  out  of  function.  Yet  the 
pseudaffective  reaction  remained  very  brisk. 

If,  therefore,  neither  the  dorsal  nor  the  ventral  column  nor 
the  gray  matter  affords  the  pathway  for  the  noci-ceptive  (algesia) 
impulses,  the  lateral  column  alone  is  left  to  them.  This  con- 
clusion is  confirmed  by  direct  experiment.  After  transection 
of  one  lateral  column  alone  the  pseudaffective  reaction  is  elicited 
from  either  lateral  half  of  the  body  behind  the  lesion;  after 
further  section  of  the  opposite  lateral  column,  all  pseudaffective 
reaction  at  once  ceases  to  be  elicitable  from  either  half  of  the 
body  behind  the  lesion.  It  is  probable  that  in  the  posterior 
thoracic  and  lumbar  segments  this  headward  path  is  that  already 
signalized  by  A.   Frohlich  and  myself^^  as  inhibiting,  under 


I 

254     REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS      [Lect.  *! 

direct  faradization,  the  rigidity  of  the   triceps  brachii  in   the  \ 
decerebrate  cat.  I 

I  We  concluded  from  our  observations  (i)  that  the  lateral  \ 
\  column  furnishes  the  headward  path  in  the  spinal  cord  for 
noci-ceptive  (algesic)  arcs;  (2)  that  each  lateral  column  con- 
veys  such  impulses  from  both  lateral  halves  of  the  body,  and  | 
somewhat  preponderantly  those  from  the  crossed  half;  and  (3)1 
that  this  is  true  for  these  arcs  whether  they  be  traced  from  | 
skin^  muscleSy  or  viscera. 

It  is  noteworthy  that  the  "  chloroform  "  or  "  ether  cry,"  that  ! 
peculiar  vocalization  emitted  by  men  and  animals  during  certain  : 
stages  of  anaesthetization,  was  often  uttered  ^'°  by  decerebrate  j 
cats  during  the  continued  administration  of  the  anaesthetic  after  ! 
decerebration.  This  vocalization  does  not  necessarily  mean  an  \ 
imperfect  anaesthetization  or  any  persistence  of  consciousness,  ; 
since  in  our  animals  the  whole  cerebrum  and  the  "  'tween  "-brain  1 
had  been  ablated  when  the  administration  of  the  vapour  still  | 
evoked  the  vocalization  typically. 

The  crying  of  the  young  infant  has  been  noticed  in  hemi-  | 
cephalic  children  to  be  strong  and  of  usual  character  even  ! 
in  total  absence  of  the  cerebrum  and  midbrain  (Sternberg  and 
Latzko^^^).  These  malformed  infants  seem  to  react  as  do  \ 
normal  of  the  same  age  to  stimuli  that,  judging  from  adult  ! 
experience,  are  unpleasant.  They  cry  or  whimper,  pucker  the  ; 
mouth,  and  retract  the  head.  The  drawing  down  of  the  angles  3 
of  the  mouth  and  the  drawing  down  of  the  lower  lip  seem  in-  | 
dicative  of  pain :  pouting  of  the  lips  —  a  mimetic  movement  i 
common  also  in  the  young  gorilla,  chimpanzee,  and  macaque  —  ■ 
seems  to  indicate  displeasure.  Nothnagel  and  others  incline  to  \ 
regard  the  optic  thalamus  as  the  seat  of  the  nerve-centres  of  mi-  • 
metic  expression.  Experiments  on  animals  and  the  observations  i 
on  hemicephalic  children  just  referred  to  seem  to  contradict  ' 
this.  But  we  must  remember  that  various  grades  of  mimetic 
movements  exist  —  and  some  seem  phylogenetically  much  older  ■ 
than  others.  The  congenital  have  to  be  distinguished  from  ] 
those  that  are  acquired.  The  mimesis  of  the  infant  is  not  that 
of  the  adult.     The  latter  may  depend  on  the  thalamic  region ;  \ 


m 


BODILY   RESONANCE   OF   EMOTIONS  255 


much  of  the  former  seems  to  be  a  reaction  for  which  neither  the 
forebrain  nor  midbrain  are  necessary.  In  the  decerebrate  cat 
we  could  never  evoke  such  mimesis  as  might,  had  the  cerebrum 
been  present,  have  been  indication  of  pleasurable  sensation. 
Never,  for  instance,  could  purring  be  elicited,  although  its  oppo- 
site, snarling,  was  obtained  so  easily.  The  decerebrate  dogs 
observed  by  Goltz  ^^  responded  to  almost  all  forms  of  skin  stim- 
ulus by  growling,  as  if  in  resentment.  Thus,  they  did  so  when 
lifted  from  their  cage  to  be  fed  each  midday.  No  mimesis 
indicative  of  pleasure  was  ever  obtained  from  them.  Pain  centres 
seem  to  lie  lower  than  pleasure  centres.  As  far  as  I  can  find 
from  reference  to  books  and  the  experience  of  colleagues, 
**  pain  "  is  unknown  as  an  aura  in  cortical  epilepsy,  or  at  least  is 
of  equivocal  occurrence.  No  region  of  the  cortex  cerebri  has 
been  assigned  to  pain.  Such  negative  evidence  gives  perhaps 
extraneous  interest  to  the  ancient  view,  represented  in  modern 
times  by  Schopenhauer,  that  pleasure  is  an  absence  of  pain. 

Bodily  resonance  of  emotions.  Some  sensations  are  neutral 
or  devoid  of  affective  tone,  while  others  are  rich  in  affective 
tone.  The  development  of  these  latter  is  closely  connected 
with  the  origin  of  the  coarser  emotions.  A  physiological  inter- 
est attaches  to  these  states  of  emotion  since  certain  reactions 
of  the  bodily  organs  are,  as  is  well  known,  characteristic  of 
them.  That  marked  reactions  of  the  nervous  arcs  regulating 
the  thoracic  and  abdominal  organs  and  the  skin  contribute 
characteristically  to  the  phenomena  of  emotion  has  been  com- 
mon knowledge  from  time  immemorial. 

To  this  bodily  resonance  of  the  emotions  has  in  recent  years 
been  assigned  by  some  authorities  a  prominent  r61e  in  the  mech- 
anism of  the  production  of  the  emotional  state  itself  in  certain 
of  the  coarser  emotions.  Instead  of  the  emotional  state  begin- 
ning, as  Ladd^i^  puts  it,  as  "  a  sort  of  nerve  storm  in  the  brain, 
whence  there  descends  an  excitement  which  causes  commotion 
in  the  viscera  and  vascular  regions  —  thus  secondarily  inducing 
an  organic  reverberation  "  —  the  view  has  been  advanced  that  the 
cerebral  and  psychological  processes  of  emotion  are  secondary 
to  an  immediate  reflex  reaction  of  vascular  and  visceral  organs 


256      REFLEXES   AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS       [Lect. 

of  the  body  suddenly  excited  by  certain  stimuli   of  peculiai 
quality. 

Of  points  where  physiology  and  psychology  touch,  the  placd 
of  one  lies  at  "  emotion."  Built  upon  sense-feeling  much  asj 
cognition  is  built  upon  sense-perception,  emotion  may  be  re-i 
garded  almost  as  a  "  feeling,"  —  a  feeling  excited,  not  by  a  simple  I 
little-elaborated  sensation,  but  by  a  group  or  train  of  ideas.  To\ 
such  compound  ideas  it  holds  relation  much  as  does  **  feeling"? 
to  certain  species  of  simple  sense-perceptions.  It  has  a  specials 
physiological  interest  in  that  certain  visceral  reactions  arei 
peculiarly  colligate  with  it.  Heart,  blood-vessels,  respiratory: 
muscles,  and  secretory  glands  take  special  and  characteristic  parti 
in  the  various  emotions.  These  viscera,  though  otherwise  re- 
mote from  the  general  play  of  psychical  process,  are  affected^ 
vividly  by  the  emotional.  Hence  many  a  picturesque  metaphor  \ 
of  proverb  and  phrase  and  name  —  "  the  heart  is  better  than  the  i 
head,"  anger  "swells  within  the  breast,"  "Richard  Coeur  de: 
Lion."  It  was  Descartes  ^  who  first  promoted  the  emotions  to« 
the  brain.  Even  last  century  Bichat  wrote,^  "  The  brain  is  the  5 
seat  of  cognition,  and  is  never  affected  by  the  emotions,  whose  ^ 
sole  seat  lies  in  the  viscera."  But  the  brain  is  now  thought  to 
be  a  factor  necessary  in  all  higher  animals  to  every  mechanism  \ 
whose  working  has  consciousness  as  an  adjunct. 

What  is  the  meaning  of  the  intimate  linkage  of  visceraL 
actions  to  psychical  states  emotional?  To  the  ordinary  day's ^ 
consciousness  in  the  healthy  individual  the  life  of  the  viscera^ 
contributes  little  at  all,  except  under  emotion.  The  perceptions  i 
of  the  normal  consciousness  are  rather  those  of  outlook  upon 
the  circumambient  universe  than  inlook  into  the  microcosm  of  the^ 
"  material  me."  Yet  heightened  beating  of  the  heart,  blanching  or  i 
flushing  of  the  blood-vessels,  the  pallor  of  fear,  the  blush  of  shame,  \ 
the  Rabelaisian  effect  of  fright  upon  the  bowel,  the  secretion  by  the  \ 
lacrymal  gland  in  grief,  all  these  are  prominent  characters  in  the  1 
pantomime  of  natural  emotion.  Visceral  disturbance  is  evidently  3 
a  part  of  the  corporeal  expression  of  emotion.  The  explanation  \ 
is  a  particular  case  in  the  problem  of  movements  of  expression  1 
in  general.     The  hypothesis  of  evolution  afforded  a  new  van-  \ 


VII]  EMOTIONAL  EXPRESSION  257 

tage  point  for  study  of  that  question.  The  bodily  expressions 
of  the  "  coarser  or  animal  emotions  "  are  largely  common  to 
man  and  higher  animals.  This  point  of  view  is  exemplified 
by  Darwin's  argument  ^  concerning  the  contraction  of  the 
muscles  round  the  eyes  during  screaming.  "  Children,  when 
wanting  food  or  when  suffering  in  any  way,  cry  out  loudly, 
as  do  the  young  of  most  animals,  partly  as  a  call  to  their 
parents  for  aid,  and  partly  from  any  great  exertion  serving 
as  relief.  Prolonged  screaming  inevitably  leads  to  the  engorg- 
ing of  the  blood-vessels  of  the  eye;  and  this  will  have  led 
at  first  consciously  and  at  last  habitually  to  the  contraction 
of  the  muscles  round  the  eyes  in  order  to  protect  them."  ^  Her- 
bert Spencer  wrote  :^^  "  Fear,  when  strong,  expresses  itself  in 
cries,  in  efforts  to  hide  or  escape,  in  palpitations  and  tremblings ; 
and  these  are  just  the  manifestations  which  would  accompany  an 
actual  experience  of  the  evil  feared.  The  destructive  passions 
are  shown  in  a  general  tension  of  the  muscular  system,  in  gnash- 
ing of  the  teeth  and  protrusion  of  the  claws,  in  dilated  eyes  and 
nostrils,  in  growls :  and  these  are  weaker  forms  of  the  actions 
that  accompany  the  killing  of  prey."  In  short,  the  bodily  ex- 
pressions of  emotion  are  instinctive  actions  reminiscent  of  ances- 
tral ways  of  life. 

They  must  have  an  explanation  the  same  in  kind  as  that  of 
other  instinctive  movement.  There  is  no  real  break  between 
man  and  brute  even  in  the  matter  of  mental  endowment.  The 
instinctive  bodily  expressions  of  emotion  arose,  in  the  opinion 
of  those  quoted  above,  as  attitudes  and  movements  useful  to 
the  animal  for  defence,  escape,  seizure,  embrace,  etc.  These  as 
survivals  have  become  .symbolic  for  states  of  mind.  Hence 
an  intelligible  nexus  between  the  muscular  attitude,  the  pose 
of  feature,  etc.,  and  the  emotional  state  of  mind.  But  between 
action  of  the  viscera  and  the  psychical  state  the  nexus  is  less 
obvious.  This  latter  connection  adds  a  difficult  corollary  to 
the  general  problem. 

The  fact  of  the  connection  is  on  all  hands  admitted,  but  as  to 
the  manner  of  it  opinion  is  at  issue.  Does  (i)  the  psychical  part 
of  the  emotion  arise  and  its  correlate  nervous  action  then  excite 

17 


258         REFLEXES   AS   ADAPTED    REACTIONS  [Lect.    I 

h 

the  viscera?  Or  (2)  does  the  same  stimulus  which  excites  the 
mind  excite  concurrently  and  per  se  the  nervous  centres  ruling 
the  viscera?  Or  (3)  does  the  stimulus  which  is  the  exciting 
cause  of  the  emotion  act  first  on  the  nervous  centres  ruling  the 
viscera,  and  their  reaction  then  generate  visceral  sensations; 
and  do  these  latter,  laden  with  affective  quality  as  we  know  they 
will  be,  induce  the  emotion  of  the  mind  ?  On  the  first  of  the 
three  hypotheses  the  visceral  reaction  will  be  secondary  to 
the  psychical,  on  the  second  the  two  will  be  collateral  and 
concurrent,  on  the  third  the  psychical  process  will  be  secondary 
to  the  visceral. 

To  examine  the  last  supposition  first.     It  is  a  view  which  in  f^ 
recent    years    has    won    notable   adherents.      Professor   James  i 
writes :  124  ♦  «.  q^.  natural  way  of  thinking  about  these  coarser  | 
emotions  (^e.g.  "grief,  fear,  rage,  love")  is  that  the  mental  per-  l 
ception  of  some  fact  excites  the  mental   affection  called   the  \ 
emotion,  and  that  this  latter  state  of  mind  gives  rise  to  the  bodily 
expression.       My  theory,   on  the    contrary,  is  that   the  bodily 
changes  follow  directly  the  perception  of  the  exciting  facty  and 
that  our  feeling  of  the  same  changes  as  they  occur  IS  the  emotion^ 
"  Every  one  of  the  bodily  changes,  whatsoever  it  be,  is  PEL  T  ^^ 
acutely  or  obscurely,  the  moment  it  occurs.     If  the  reader  has  i 
never  paid  attention  to  this  matter,  he  will  be  both  interested  \ 
and  astonished  to  learn  how  many  different  local  bodily  feelings  | 
he  can  detect  in  himself  as  characteristic  of  his  various  emo-   1 
tional  moods."     "  If  we  fancy  some  strong  emotion  and  then  try 
to  abstract  from  our  consciousness  of  it  all  the  feelings  of  its   \ 
bodily  symptoms  we  find  we  have  nothing  left  behind,  no  *  mind-    , 
stuff"'  out  of  which  the  emotion  can  be  constituted,  and  that  a    ^ 
cold   and   neutral   state   of  intellectual   perception   is   all   that 
remains."     "  If  I   were  to   become   corporeally   anaesthetic,  I 
should  be  excluded  from  the  life  of  the  affections,  harsh  and 
tender  alike,  and  drag  an  existence  of  merely  cognitive  or  intel- 
lectual form." 

Professor  Lange  ^^*  traces  the  whole  psycho-physiology  of 
amotion  to  certain  excitations  of  the  vasomotor  centre.     For  him, 

*  The  italics  and  emphasizing  capitals  are  quoted  as  in  the  original. 


1 


I]  PHYSIOLOGICAL  REACTIONS  259 

as  for  Professor  James,  the  emotion  is  the  outcome  and  not  the 
cause  or  the  concomitant  of  the  organic  reaction ;  but  for  him  the 
foundation  and  corner-stone  of  the  organic  reaction  is  as  to  physi- 
ological quality  vascular,  namely,  vasomotor.  Emotion  is  an  out- 
come of  vasomotor  reaction  to  stimuli  of  a  particular  kind.  This 
stimulus  induces  a  vasomotor  action  in  viscera,  skin,  and  brain. 
The  change  thus  induced  in  the  circulatory  condition  of  these 
organs  induces  changes  in  the  actions  of  the  organs  themselves, 
and  these  latter  evoke  sensations  which  constitute  the  essen- 
tial part  of  emotion.  It  is  by  excitation  of  the  vasomotor 
centre,  therefore,  that  the  exciting  cause,  whatever  it  chance 
to  be,  of  emotion  produces  the  organic  phenomena  which  as 
felt  constitute  for  Lange  the  whole  essence  of  emotion.  The 
teaching  of  Professor  Sergi  ^^'  ^""  closely  approaches  to  that  of 
Lange. 

The  views  of  James,  Lange,  and  Sergi  have  common  to  them 
this,  that  the  psychical  process  of  emotion  is  secondary  to  a 
discharge  of  nervous  impulses  into  the  vascular  and  visceral 
organs  of  the  body  suddenly  excited  by  certain  peculiar  stimuli, 
and  that  it  depends  upon  the  reaction  of  those  organs.  Pro- 
fessor James's  position  in  the  matter  is,  however,  not  wholly  like 
that  of  Professor  Lange.  In  the  first  place,  he  does  not  consider 
vasomotor  reaction  to  be  primary  to  all  the  other  organic  and 
visceral  disturbances  that  carry  in  their  train  the  psychological 
appanage  of  emotion ;  and  Professor  Sergi,  though  more  nearly 
in  harmony  with  Lange,  agrees  with  James  so  far.  In  the  sec- 
ond place,  Professor  James  seems  to  distinctly  include  other 
"  motor  "  sensations  and  centripetal  impulses  from  musculature 
other  than  visceral  and  vascular,  among  those  which  casually 
contribute  to  emotion.  Thirdly,  he  urges  his  theory  as  one 
completely  competent  only  for  the  **  coarser  "  emotions,  among 
which  he  instances  **  fear,  anger,  love,  and  grief."  For  Lange 
and  Sergi  the  basis  of  apparition  of  all  feeling  and  emotion  is 
physiological,  visceral,  and  organic,  and  has  its  seat  for  the 
former  authority  exclusively,  and  for  the  latter  eminently,  in  the 
vasomotor  system. 

To  obtain  some  test  of  this  view  is  not  difficult  by  experi- 


26o      REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS     [Lect. 

ment.^^^  Appropriate  spinal  and  vagal  transection  removes 
completely  and  immediately  the  sensation  of  the  viscera  and  of 
all  the  skin  and  muscles  behind  the  shoulder  (Fig.  70).  The 
procedure  at  the  same  time  cuts  from  connection  with  the  organs 
of  consciousness  the  whole  of  the  circulatory  apparatus  of  the 
body.  I  have  had  under  observation  dogs  in  which  this  has 
been  carried  out.     I  will  cite  an  animal  selected  because  of 


•  I 


Figure  70.  —  Diagram  to  indicate  the  extent  of  the  parts  still  retaining  sensitivity  after 

combined  spinal  and  vagosympathetic  nerve  sections  described  in  the  text.     The  extent  j 

of   skin   surface  left  sentient  is  delimited  by  the  continuous  (not  dotted)  lines  in  the  i 

figure.     The  limit  of  "deep,"  i.e.  muscular,  articular,  etc.,  sensitivity  also  corresponds  j 

with  this  line.     But  the  limit  to  which  the   respiratory  and  alimentary  tracts  still  re-  i 

tained  sensation  is  shown  by  dotted  outlines  of  the  larynx  and  upper  part  of  oesophagus.  ] 
From  anatomical  data  it  is  presumed  that  the  trachea  and  oesophagus  had  been  de- 
prived of  all  sensitivity  somewhere  about  those  levels.    The  curved  line  behind  the  chest 
indicates  the  diaphragm  as  the  only  muscle  behind  the  shoulder  still  retaining  afferent 

nerves.  I 

markedly   emotional    temperament.      Affectionate   toward    the  J 

laboratory  attendants,  one  of  whom  had  her  in  charge,  toward  ;: 

some  persons  and  toward  several  inmates  of  the  animal  house  ; 

she  frequently  showed  violent  anger.     Her  ebullitions  of  rage  '\ 

were   sudden.     Their  expression  accorded  with  a  description  | 
furnished   by  Darwin.^     Besides   the    utterance  of  the   growl, 

"  the  ears  are  pressed  closely  backwards,  and  the  upper  lip  is  ^ 

retracted  out  of  the  way  of  the  teeth,  especially  of  the  canines."  | 

The  mouth  was  slightly  opened  and  lifted,  the  eyelids  widely  ^ 

parted,  the  pupils  dilated.     The  hair  along  the  mid-dorsum,  fi-om  !j 


VII]  ANGER  261 

close  behind  the  head  to  a  point  more  than  half-way  down  the 
trunk,  became  rough  and  bristling. 

The  reduction  of  the  field  of  sensation  in  this  animal  by  the 
procedure  above  mentioned  produced  no  obvious  diminution  of 
her  emotional  character.  Her  anger,  her  joy,  her  disgust,  and 
when  provocation  arose,  her  fear,  remained  as  evident  as  ever. 
Her  joy  at  the  approach  or  notice  of  the  attendant,  her  rage  at 
the  intrusion  of  a  cat  with  which  she  was  unfriendly,  remained 
as  active  and  thorough.  But  among  the  signs  expressive  of  rage 
the  bristling  of  the  coat  along  the  back  no  longer  occurred.  On 
the  other  hand,  the  eyes  were  well  opened  and  the  pupils  dis- 
tinctly dilated  in  the  paroxysm  of  anger.  Since  by  the  transec- 
tion the  brain  had  been  shut  out  from  discharging  impulses  via 
the  cervical  sympathetic  the  dilatation  of  pupil  may  have  occurred 
by  inhibition  of  the  action  of  the  oculomotor  centre. 

The  coming  of  a  visitor  whose  advent  months  before  had 
elicited  violent  anger,  again  provoked  an  exhibition  of  wrath 
significant  as  ever.  The  expression  was  that  of  aggressive  rage. 
The  animal  followed  each  movement  of  the  stranger  as  though 
of  an  opponent,  growling  viciously.  A  cat  with  which  she  was 
never  friendly,  and  a  monkey  new  to  the  laboratory,  approach- 
ing too  near  the  kennel,  excited  similar  outbursts.  No  doubt 
was  left  in  our  minds  that  sudden  attacks  of  violent  anger  were 
still  easily  excited.  But  she  also  gave  evidence  daily  that  she 
had  the  accession  of  joyous  pleasure  and  delight  she  had  always 
shown  at  the  approach  of  the  attendant  the  first  thing  of  a  morn- 
ing, or  at  feeding  time,  or  when  caressed  by  him,  or  encouraged 
by  his  voice. 

Few  dogs,  even  when  very  hungry,  can  be  prevailed  upon  to 
touch  dog's  flesh  as  food.  Almost  all  turn  from  it  with  signs  of 
repugnance  and  dislike.  I  had  strictly  refrained  from  testing 
this  animal  previously  with  regard  to  disgust  at  dog's  flesh 
off"ered  in  her  food.  Flesh  was  given  her  daily  in  a  bowl  of 
milk,  and  this  she  took  with  relish.  The  meat  was  cut  into 
pieces  rather  larger  than  the  lumps  of  sugar  usual  for  the  break- 
fast table.  It  was  generally  horse-flesh,  sometimes  ox-flesh. 
We  proceeded  to  the  observation  thus:   the  bowl  was  placed 


262        REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS    [Lect^ 

ft 

by  the  attendant  in  the  corner  of  the  stall  with  milk  and  meat  it^ 

every  way  as  usual ;  but  the  meat  was  flesh  from  a  dog  killed  on 
the  previous  day.  Our  animal  eagerly  drew  itself  toward  the 
food ;  it  had  seen  the  other  dogs  fed,  and  evidently  was  itself 
hungry.  Its  muzzle  had  almost  dipped  into  the  milk  before  it 
suddenly  seemed  to  find  something  there  amiss.  It  hesitated, 
moved  its  muzzle  about  above  the  milk,  made  a  venture  to  take  a 
piece  of  the  meat,  but  before  actually  seizing  it  stopped  short  and 
withdrew  again  from  it.  Finally,  after  some  further  examination 
of  the  contents  of  the  bowl  (it  usually  commenced  by  taking  out 
and  eating  the  pieces  of  meat),  without  touching  them,  the 
creature  turned  away  from  the  bowl  and  withdrew  itself  to 
the  opposite  side  of  the  cage.  Some  minutes  later,  under 
encouragement  from  us  to  try  the  food  again,  it  returned  to  the 
bowl.  The  same  hesitant  display  of  conflicting  desire  and  dis- 
gust was  once  more  gone  through.  The  bowl  was  then  removed 
by  the  attendant,  emptied,  washed,  and  horse-flesh  similarly 
prepared  and  placed  in  a  fresh  quantity  of  milk  was  offered  in 
it  to  the  animal.  The  animal  once  more  drew  itself  toward  the 
bowl,  and  this  time  began  to  eat  the  meat,  soon  emptying  the 
dish.  To  press  the  flesh  upon  our  animal  was  of  no  real  avail 
on  any  occasion ;  the  coaxing  only  succeeded  in  getting  her  to, 
as  it  were,  re-examine  but  not  to  touch  the  morsels.  The 
impression  made  on  all  of  us  by  the  dog's  behaviour  was  that 
something  in  the  dog's  flesh  was  repulsive  to  her,  and  excited  dis- 
gust unconquerable  by  ordinary  hunger.  Some  odour  attaching 
to  the  flesh  seemed  the  source  of  its  recognition. 

It  would  be  instructive  for  judging  the  part  played  by  the 
cerebral  hemisphere  in  the  reactions  of  coarser  emotion  did  we 
know  whether  repugnance  to  dog's  flesh  as  food  would  be  ex- 
hibited by  a  dog  after  ablation  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres. 
Even  the  primitive  emotions  seem  to  involve  perception  —  seem 
little  other  than  sense-perceptions  richly  suffused  with  affec- 
tive tone.  Goltz's^^  dogs  after  ablation  of  the  hemispheres 
evinced  signs  of  hunger,  namely  restlessness  when  their  feed- 
ing hour  was  deferred.  When  a  little  quinine  (bitter)  was  ' 
added   to   the  sop   of  meat   and  milk  the  morsels  taken  into 


VII]  DISGUST  263 

the  mouth  were  at  once  rejected.  No  inducement  or  scold- 
ing modified  this  unfailing  and  unhesitating  rejection.  Goltz 
adds  that  he  threw  to  his  own  house  dog  a  piece  of  the 
same  doctored  meat.  The  creature  wagged  its  tail  and  took  it 
eagerly,  then  pulled  a  wry  face,  and  hesitated,  astonished.  But 
on  a  look  of  encouragement  from  its  master  the  dog  swallowed 
it.  He  overcame  his  instinctive  rejection  of  it,  and  thus,  as  Goltz 
remarks,  by  his  self-control  gave  proof  of  the  intact  cerebrum 
he  possessed. 

Fear  appeared  clearly  elicitable  (as  also  in  dogs  with  spinal 
cervical  transection  only.  Fig.  71).  The  attendant  approaching 
from  another  room  of  which  the  door  stood  open,  chid  the  dog 
in  high  scolding  tones.  The  creature's  head  sank,  her  gaze 
turned  away  from  her  advancing  master,  and  her  face  seemed  to 
betray  dejection  and  anxiety.  The  respiration  became  unquiet, 
but  the  pulse  never  changed  its  rate. 

In  the  dog,  after  transection  of  the  spinal  cord,  the  regions 
of  the  body  which  have  been  thus  made  purely  "  spinal "  con- 
tinue their  life  in  many  respects  normally.  The  hairy  "  coat " 
changes  in  spring.  The  oestral  periods  recur  even  when  the 
transection  is  performed  in  puppyhood,  and  altogether  headward 
of  the  spinal  origin  of  the  sympathetic  system,  e.g.  at  the  cervi- 
cal segment.  Goltz  ^  observed  successful  impregnation  and 
parturition,  and  suckling  completed  without  obvious  abnormal- 
ity. In  my  own  observations  2^^*  the  natural  instinct  of  the 
female  toward  the  male  at  oestrum  was  seen  indubitably  dis- 
played after  the  spinal  cord  had  been  transected  in  the  cervical 
region  more  than  a  year  previously. 

It  may  be  objected  to  these  experiments  that  although  the 
animals  expressed  emotion  they  may  yet  have  felt  none.  Had 
their  expression  been  unaccompanied  by,  and  had  they  not  led  on 
to,  trains  of  acts  logically  consonant  with  their  expressed  emotion, 
that  objection  would  have  weight.  Where  ^q  fades  of  anger  is 
followed  by  actions  of  advance  and  attack  with  all  appearance  of 
set  purpose,  I  find  it  difficult  to  think  that  the  perception  initiat- 
ing the  wrathful  expression  should  bring  in  sequel  angry  conduct 
and  yet  have  been  impotent  to  produce  "  angry  feeling." 


264       REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS    [Lect. 


I 


-i 


Figure  71.  —  Record  of  the  arterial  pressure  in  a  dog  forty-one  days  after  spinal  transection 
at  the  7th  cervical  segment.  The  arterial  pressure  is  high  and  good  in  spite  of  the  tran- 
section, the  period  of  vasomotor  shock  having  passed  by.  For  the  short  period  marked 
by  the  signal  the  noise  of  the  vibrator  of  an  inductorium  sounded  and  was  heard  by  the 
animal.  The  point  of  the  signal  marked  nearly  8  mm.  further  to  the  right  than  did  the 
kymograph  pen.  The  inhibition  of  the  heart  is  shown  by  the  oscillations  on  the  kymo- 
graph trace.  The  line  marked  "  Zero  of  B.  /*."  signifies  the  height  of  the  zero  of  the 
manometer  recording  the  arterial  pressure. 


i 


VII]    BODILY  REINFORCEMENT   OF   EMOTION      265 

W  A  weaker  point  in  such  experimentation  is  that  although  the 
visceral  and  vascular  and  much  of  the  muscular  mechanism  of 
emotional  expression  was  cut  off,  a  small  but  notable  fraction 
of  the  latter,  namely  the  facial,  still  remained  open  to  react  on 
the  centres  with  which  consciousness  is  colligate. 

Nevertheless,  in  view  of  these  observations  the  vasomotor 
theory  of  the  production  of  emotion  becomes,  I  think,  untenable, 
also  that  visceral  sensations  or  presentations  are  necessary  to 
emotion.  A  mere  remnant  of  all  the  non-projecting  or  affective 
senses  was  left,  and  yet  emotion  persisted.  If  I  understand  it 
aright,  Professor  James'  and  Lange's  theory  lays  stress  on  organic 
and  visceral  presentations,  but  re-presentations  of  the  same 
species  might  no  doubt  be  put  forward  in  their  place.  That 
would  be  a  different  matter.  To  exclude  the  latter  hypothesis, 
the  deprivation  of  vascular  and  organic  sensation  might  have  to 
date  from  a  very  early  period  of  the  individual  life.  Professor 
Lloyd  Morgan  writes  2^^*  in  respect  to  the  above  experiments, 
"  The  avenues  of  connection  were  closed  after  the  motor  and 
visceral  effects  had  played  their  part  in  the  genesis  of  the  emo- 
tion on  the  hypothesis  that  the  emotion  is  thus  generated.  Al- 
though new  presentative  data  of  this  type  were  thus  excluded, 
their  re-presentative  after-effects  in  the  situation  were  not  ex- 
cluded." But  it  is  noteworthy  that  one  of  the  dogs  under 
observation  had  been  deprived  of  its  sensation  when  only  nine 
weeks  old.  Disgust  for  dog's  flesh  could  hardly  arise  from  the 
experience  of  nine  weeks  of  puppyhood  in  the  kennel. 

We  are  forced  back  toward  the  likelihood  that  the  visceral 
expression  of  emotion  is  secondary  to  the  cerebral  action  occur- 
ring with  the  psychical  state.  There  is  a  strong  bond  between 
emotion  and  muscular  action.  Emotion  "  moves  "  us,  hence  the 
word  itself.  If  developed  in  intensity,  it  impels  toward  vigor- 
ous movement.  Every  vigorous  movement  of  the  body,  though 
its  more  obvious  instrument  be  the  skeletal  musculature  of  the 
limbs  and  trunk,  involves  also  the  less  noticeable  co-operation 
of  the  viscera,  especially  of  the  circulatory  and  respiratory. 
The  extra  demand  made  upon  the  muscles  that  move  the  frame 
involves  a  heightened  action  of  the  nutrient  organs  which  sup- 


266        REFLEXES  AS  ADAPTED   REACTIONS    [Lect. 

ply  to  the  muscles  the  material  for  their  energy.  This  increased 
action  of  the  viscera  is  colligate  with  this  activity  of  muscles. 
We  should  expect  visceral  action  to  occur  along  with  the  muscu- 
lar expression  of  emotion.  The  close  tie  between  visceral  action 
and  states  of  emotion  need  not  therefore  surprise  us. 

That  emotion  is  primarily  a  cerebral  reaction  obtains  sup- 
port from  observations  where  the  hemispheres  of  the  brain  have 
been  removed.  Goltz  observed  a  dog  kept  many  months  in 
that  condition.  It  on  no  occasion  gave  any  evidence  of  joy  or 
pleasure  in  commerce  either  with  man  or  beast.  Of  sexual 
emotion  it  never  gave  a  sign.  Anger  or  displeasure,  Goltz 
says,  it  repeatedly  expressed,  both  by  gesture  and  by  voice. 
Save  for  these  expressions  of  displeasure,  it  was  indifferent  and 
supremely  neutral  to  its  surroundings.  We  are,  of  course,  in 
observations  such  as  this,  hopelessly  cut  off  from  introspective 
help.  It  can  be  urged  that  the  expression  of  emotion  might 
be  provocable  and  nevertheless  the  psychical  emotion  remain 
absent.  On  such  an  hypothesis  the  same  stimulus  which  excited 
the  mind  must  excite  concurrently  and  per  se  motor  centres 
producing  movement  appropriate  to  an  affective  process  in  the 
mind.  This  is  not  improbable.  All  sensations  referred  to  the 
body  itself  rather  than  ittterpreted  as  qualities  of  objects  in  the 
external  worlds  tend  to  be  tinged  with  "  feeling."  Sense-organs 
which  initiate  sensations  tinged  with  feeling  tend  to  excite  motor 
centres  directly  and  imperatively.  Hence  in  animals  reduced  to 
merely  spinal  condition  stimuli  calculated  to  produce  pain 
(although,  of  course,  unable  to  do  so  in  a  spinal  animal)  evoke 
movements  appropriate  for  escape  from  or  removal  of  the  stimu- 
lus applied.  Now  "  feeling"  is  implicit  in  the  emotional  state; 
the  state  is  an  "  affective  state."  In  the  evolution  of  emotion 
the  revival  of  "  feelings "  pleasureable  and  painful  must  have 
played  a  large  part.  Hence  the  close  relation  of  emotion  with 
sense-organs  that  can  initiate  bodily  pain  or  pleasure,  and  hence 
its  connection  with  impulsive  or  instinctive  movement.  There 
is  no  wide  interval  between  the  reflex  movement  of  the  spinal 
dog  whose  foot  attempts  to  scratch  away  an  irritant  applied  to 
its  back  —  both  leg  and  back   absolutely  detached  from  con- 


I 


VII]  EMOTIONAL   REACTION  267 

sciousness  —  and  the  reaction  of  the  decerebrate  dog  that  turns 
and  growls  and  bites  at  the  fingers  holding  his  hind  foot  too 
roughly.  In  the  former  case  the  motor  reaction  occurs,  although 
the  mind  is  not  even  aware  of  the  stimulus,  far  less  percipient 
of  it  as  an  irritant.  The  action  occurs,  and  plays  the  panto- 
mime of  feeling;  but  no  feeling  comes  to  pass.  In  the  latter 
case  the  motor  reaction  occurs  and  is  expressive  of  emotion ; 
but  it  is  probably  the  reaction  of  an  organic  machine  which 
can  be  started  working,  though  the  mutilation  precludes  the 
psychosis. 

And  with  the  gesture  and  the  attitude  will  occur  the  visceral 
concomitant.  It  would  be  consonant  with  what  we  know  of 
reflex  action,  if  the  spur  that  started  the  muscular  expression 
should  simultaneously  and  of  itself  initiate  also  the  visceral 
adjunct  reaction.  It  is  almost  impossible  to  believe  that  with 
the  mere  stump  of  brain  that  remained  to  Goltz's  dog  there 
could  be  any  elaboration  of  a  percept.  All  trace  of  memory 
seemed  lacking  to  the  creature.  Yet  though  not  evincing 
other  emotion,  anger  it  showed  as  far  as  expression  can  yield 
such  revelation.  Fear,  joy,  affection  seem,  therefore,  in  the  ex- 
perience of  this  skilled  observer  of  animal  behaviour,  to  demand 
higher  nervous  organization  than  does  anger.  Be  that  as  it  may, 
the  retention  of  its  expression  by  Goltz's  dog  indicates  that  by 
"  retrogradation  "  the  complex  movement  of  expression  has  in 
certain  emotions  passed  into  a  simpler  reflex-act.  Under  the 
canalizing  force  of  habit  the  determining  motives  become,  even 
in  impulsive  acts,  weaker  and  more  transient.  The  external 
stimulus  originally  aroused  a  strongly  affective  group  of  ideas, 
which  operated  as  a  motive,  but  now  it  causes  a  discharge  of  the 
act  before  it  can  be  apprehended  as  an  idea.  The  impulsive 
movement  of  a  "  lower,"  "  coarser,"  so-called  "  animal  "  emotion, 
has  in  this  case  become  an  automatic  reflex  no  longer  neces- 
sarily combined  with  the  psychical  state  whence  it  arose,  of 
which  it  is  normally  at  once  the  adjunct  and  the  symbol. 

In  view  of  these  general  considerations  and  of  the  above 
experiments,  we  may  with  James  accept  visceral  and  organic 
sensations  and  the  memories  and  associations  of  them  as  con- 


268         REFLEXES   AS   ADAPTED   REACTIONS   [Lect. 

tributory  to  primitive  emotions,  but  we  must  regard  them  as| 
reinforcing  rather  than  initiating  the  psychosis.  Organic  and| 
vascular  reaction,  though  not  the  actual  excitant  of  emotion, 
strengthen  it.  This  is  the  kernel  of  the  old  contention  about 
actuality  of  emotion  in  the  art  of  the  artist.  Hamlet's  descrip- 
tion of  the  actor  as  really  moved  by  his  expression  may  be 
accepted  as  an  answer. 

Conversely,  as  Lloyd  Morgan  2^^*  writes,  "  Whatever  be  the 
exact  psychological  nature  of  the  emotions,  it  may  be  regarded 
as  certain  that  they  introduce  into  the  conscious  situation  ele- 
ments which  contribute  not  a  little  to  the  energy  of  behaviour." 
A  feeling  of  pain  and  a  protective  reflex  movement  —  either 
of  defence  or  escape  —  are  concurrent  in  the  reaction  of  an 
animal  to  a  hurtful  stimulus  of  the  skin.  Reflexes  to  which 
emotion  is  adjunct  are  not  only  prepotent  (Lect.  VI)  but  are 
imperative,  that  is,  volition  cannot  easily  suppress  them.  Now, 
the  morphological  disposition  of  the  nervous  channels  is  such 
that  the  physiologist  can,  by  suitable  severance  of  the  spinal 
path  to  the  brain,  sunder  the  reflex  movement  from  the  sensa- 
tion, leaving  the  former  effect  but  perforce  annulling  the  latter. 
The  former  is,  however,  in  absence  of  the  latter,  not  left  un- 
altered; it  is  abnormally  reduced,  especially  in  duration  {v. 
supra,  p.  252).  The  pseudaffective  reactions  indicative  of  re- 
sentment and  defence  are,  after  ablation  of  the  cerebral  cortex, 
short-lived,  the  simulacra  of  mere  flashes  of  mimetic  passion.  No 
cerebral  reverberation  descends  to  prolong  and  develop  further 
the  protective  movement  set  going  as  a  spinal  reflex.  This  con- 
trasts strongly  with  the  fairly  normal  course  that  the  headward 
part  of  the  reflex,  after  loss  of  its  vascular  and  visceral  fields, 
runs.  The  difference  argues  that  the  reverberation  from  the 
trunk,  limbs,  and  viscera  counts  for  relatively  little,  even  in  the 
primitive  emotions  of  the  dog,  as  compared  with  the  cerebral 
reverberation  to  which  is  adjunct  the  psychical  component  of 
emotional  reaction. 


VIII]  FUNCTIONAL  TOPOGRAPHY  269 


LECTURE   VIII 

SOME   ASPECTS    OF   THE    REACTIONS   OF    THE    MOTOR 

CORTEX 

Argument:  Remarkable  that  electrical  stimuli  applied  to  the  organ  of 
mentality  yield  with  regularity  certain  localized  movements  from 
certain  restricted  areas  of  its  surface.  Functional  topography  of 
"motor"  cortex  in  the  chimpanzee,  orang-utan,  and  gorilla.  The 
cerebral  fissures,  not  functional  boundaries.  The  anthropoid  ape  has 
a  direct  pyramidal  tract  like  that  of  man.  Recovery  of  function  not 
due  to  symmetrical  part  of  opposite  hemisphere  taking  on  supple- 
mental work.  Inhibition  as  elicitable  from  the  cortex.  Reciprocal 
innervation  of  antagonistic  eye-muscles.  Reciprocal  inhibition  in 
other  muscular  groups.  Seat  of  the  inhibition  subcortical  in  these 
cases.  Reciprocal  innervation  in  willed  movements.  Preponderant 
representation  in  the  "  motor  "  cortex  of  the  same  movements  as  are 
preponderantly  elicitable  as  local  reflexes  from  the  cord  and  bulb. 
Scanty  representation  of  certain  movements  as  cortical  and  local 
spinal  reactions  alike.  Appearance  under  strychnine  and  tetanus 
toxin  of  these  movements  reversing  the  normal  direction  of  the 
preponderance.  This  due  to  these  agents  transmuting  reciprocal 
inhibition  into  excitation.  Decerebrate  rigidity.  A  system  of  tonic 
innen^ation  in  action.  Strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  augment  this 
innervation.  Hughlings  Jackson's  "co-operative  antagonism"  of 
paired  systems  of  innervation,  one  tonic,  the  other  phasic.  De- 
cerebrate rigidity  and  hemiplegic  rigidity.  The  relation  of  the 
cortex  to  receptor  organs ;  the  pre-emiment  representation  in  it  of 
the  *'  distance-receptors." 

We  shall  now  venture  a  glance  at  certain  reactions  of  the 
cerebral  hemisphere  itself;  our  survey  must  be  circumscript 
for  several  reasons.  By  use  of  such  methods  as  we  are  em- 
ploying, artificial  excitation  and  so  on,  and  under  such  ob- 
servations as  these  allow,  namely  the  initiation  under  narcosis 
of  muscular  movements  or  the  recording  of  their  immediate 
defects  from  normal  movement,  little  light  is  given  in  regard 
to  much  that  goes  on  in  an  organ  whose  chief  function 
is    mentality   itself.      Our    expectation    must    be   modest,   for 


b 


270     REACTIONS  OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX    [Lect. 

modest  assuredly  must  be  the  achievement  reached  by  such 
means  in  a  problem  of  such  a  nature.  The  very  poverty  of  the 
achievement  is  itself  an  indication  that  the  methods  pursued  by 
the  physiologist  successfully  in  other  spheres  of  his  study  are 
here  confronted  with  problems  to  which  they  are  far  less 
suited.  It  is  not  that  I  esteem  lightly  the  labours  of  the  many 
distinguished  workers  in  this  field.  As  far  as  the  methods 
referred  to  can  avail,  it  is  to  the  skill  with  which  they  have 
been  used  that  we  owe  what  knowledge  we  have  of  the  topo- 
graphical representation  of  movement  in  the  various  fields  of 
cerebral  cortex.  We  have  only  to  remember  how  much  more 
numerous  the  physiological  facts  concerning  the  cerebral  cortex 
are  to-day  than  prior  to  the  experiments  of  Hitzig  and  Fritsch*^ 
and  of  Ferrier,^^  following  on  the  observations  of  Broca,^ 
Hughlings  Jackson,^  and  Bastian.*^  Experiment  had  failed  to 
get  evidence  of  localization  of  function  in  the  cortex  of  the 
hemispheres,  though  in  microscopic  structure  that  great  sheet 
of  gray  matter  presents  such  similarity  to  nervous  formations 
regarded  as  nerve-centres  elsewhere.  Progress  of  knowledge  in 
regard  to  the  nervous  system  has  been  indissolubly  linked  with 
determination  of  localization  of  function  in  it.  This  has  been 
so  from  the  time  of  the  Bell  ^^-Magendie  ^^  discovery  of  the  dif- 
ference of  function  in  the  two  spinal  roots,  and  Flourens'  ^^  de- 
limitation of  the  respiratory  centre  in  the  bulb.  The  discovery 
of  localization  of  function  in  parts  of  the  cortex  has  given  the 
knowledge  which  now  supplies  to  the  student  charts  of  the 
functional  topography  of  the  brain  much  as  maps  of  continents 
are  supplied  in  a  geographical  atlas.  The  student  looking  over 
the  political  map  of  a  continent  may  little  realize  the  complexity 
of  the  populations  and  states  so  simply  represented.  We 
looking  at  the  brain  chart  of  the  text-book  may  never  forget 
the  unspeakable  complexity  of  the  reactions  thus  rudely  sym- 
bolized and  spatially  indicated. 

If  we  may  be  allowed  an  a  priori  consideration  it  is  this,  — 
that  although  it  is  not  surprising  that  such  territorial  subdivision 
of  function  should  exist  in  the  cerebral  cortex,  it  is  surprising 
that   by   our   relatively   imperfect   artifices  for  stimulation  we 


VIII]  ANTHROPOID   MOTOR   CORTEX  271 

should  be  able  to  obtain  clear  evidence  thereof.  The  neurone 
chains  that  together  build  up  the  nervous  system  are  in  the 
architecture  of  that  system  so  arranged  that  the  longest  of  them 
all  tend  to  pass  through  the  cerebral  cortex.  Every  increase 
in  the  number  of  links  composing  a  nerve-cell  chain  seems  to 
increase  greatly  the  uncertainty  of  its  reactions  under  artificial 
excitation.  With  increase  in  number  of  links  goes  increase  in 
numbers  of  side  branches  and  connections.  The  difficulty  of 
getting  long  chains  of  nerve-cells  to  react  in  a  regular  way  under 
artificial  stimulation  seems  greatly  enhanced  by  the  multiplica- 
tion of  the  side  connections.  The  momentary  condition  of  any 
cell-chain  is  in  part  a  function  of  the  condition  at  the  moment 
of  all  the  other  cell-chains  with  which  it  is  connected.  The 
cortex  cerebri  might  therefore  well  have  been  expected  to  yield 
under  artificial  stimulation  only  extraordinarily  inconstant  results. 
To  Hitzig  and  Fritsch,  and  to  Ferrier,  we  owe  the  pregnant 
demonstration  that  as  regards  the  motor  region  this  expectation 
is  not  well  founded. 

It  is  only  of  the  reactions  of  the  Rolandic  area  of  the  cortex 
that  I  shall  venture  to  speak.  Ferrier  showed  that  the  applica- 
tion of  faradic  currents  to  that  cortex  excites  with  great  regu- 
larity movements  which  vary  in  distribution  as  the  electrodes 
are  moved  from  place  to  place,  but  remain  within  limits  constant 
under  repeated  application  of  the  stimulus  to  any  one  and  the 
same  spot.  Ferrier's  mode  of  indicating  the  topographical 
arrangement  of  the  reactions  he  obtained  is  seen  in  his  well- 
known  diagrams  of  the  cortex.  His  motor  centres,  as  he 
termed  them,  were  marked  in  his  figures  by  circular  areas. 
'•  The  areas  have  no  exact  line  of  demarcation  from  each 
other,  and  where  they  adjoin  stimulation  is  apt  to  produce 
conjointly  the  effect  peculiar  to  each."  ^^  He  showed  these 
motor  centres  to  extend  forward  over  the  frontal  lobe,  produc- 
ing there  movements  of  the  eyeballs.  Regarding  their  exten- 
sion round  and  over  the  upper  edge  of  the  hemisphere  and 
down  upon  the  mesial  surface  he  noted  them  in  the  marginal 
convolution.  "This  convolution  in  the  parieto- frontal  region 
gave   rise  to    movements   of  the   head    and  limbs   apparently 


272      REACTIONS   OF  THE  MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect.  ! 

similar  to  those  already  obtained  by  stimulation  of  the  corre-  ; 
sponding  regions  on  the  external  surface."  ^^ 

This  original  research  by  Ferrier  ranks  among  the  classics  ^ 
of  experimental  neurology  and  physiology.  It  has  been  followed  \ 
by  a  number  of  kindred  contributions  from  workers  whose  names  { 
are  familiar  to  us  all,  — Albertoni,  Schafer,  Munk,  Luciani,  Tarn-  i 
burini,  Paneth,  Beevor,  Horsley,  Mott,  Ballance,  Mann,  and  others,  j 
The  detailed  knowledge  of  the  localization  has  been  largely  ; 
based  on  the  cerebral  cortex  of  the  common  ape,  the  macaque,  j 
It  was  an  interesting  step  further  when  Beevor  and  Horsley  ^^7  \ 
published  observations  on  the  localization  of  the  motor  functions  , 
in  the  central  cortex  of  an  orang-utan.  Their  experiment  ] 
long  remained  the  single  one  for  which  an  anthropoid  species  ; 
had  been  laid  under  contribution.  It  exercised  a  notable  influ- 
ence on  the  scheme  of  motor  localization  adopted  as  probably  ' 
obtaining  in  the  brain  of  man. 

Of  the  three  or  four  species  of  anthropoid  apes  that  are  ■ 
known,  most  authorities  agree  that  it  is  the  gorilla  which  pos-  \ 
sesses  the  most  highly  developed  cerebrum  ;  next  to  it  probably  ■ 
stands  the  chimpanzee,  and  a  little  below  the  chimpanzee  comes 
Simia  satyrus,  the  orang-utan.  But  there  are  great  individual  \ 
differences,  and  the  simpler  examples  of  chimpanzee  brains  seem  \ 
inferior  in  development  to  well-developed  examples  of  the  brain  i 
of  the  orang.  I 

A.  S.  Griinbaum^^^^  and  myself  have  obtained  observa-  ' 
tions  on  cerebral  localization  in  the  several  species  of  anthropoid  ; 
apes.  In  the  chimpanzee  the  scheme  of  topography  we  find  ; 
existent  is  illustrated  by  the  accompanying  Figures  72  and  73. 

The  so-called  motor  area  occupies  unbrokenly  the  whole  ' 
length  of  the  precentral  convolution,  and  in  most  places  the  ^ 
greater  part  or  the  whole  of  its  width.  It  extends  into  the  depth  : 
of  the  central  sulcus,  occupying  the  anterior  wall,  and  in  some  \ 
places  the  floor,  and  in  some  extends  even  into  the  deeper  ' 
part  of  the  posterior  wall  of  the  fissure.  We  have  examined  ^ 
more  than  forty  hemispheres,  but  have  never  found  the  motor 
area  extend  indubitably  to  the  free  face  of  the  post-central  \ 
convolution.      This   deUmitation   agrees   remarkably   with   the  ; 


VIII]  ANTHROPOID   MOTOR   CORTEX  273 

original  results  obtained  by  Hitzig^*  on  the  brain  of  the  monkey, 
Itrnuus  rhesus. 

At  the  upper  mesial  edge  of  the  hemisphere  the  motor  area 
extends  round  and  down  upon  the  mesial  face  of  the  hemisphere, 
but  we  have  not  found  it  reach  the  calloso-marginal  fissure. 
The  anterior  limit  of  the  motor  region  is  in  great  part  not  coin- 
cident with  any  fissure.  The  front  portion  of  the  region  usually 
dips  into  and  across  the  upper  part  of  the  superior  precentral 
fissure,  and  lower  down  it  not  infrequently  dips  into  the  inferior 
precentral  fissure.  Occasionally  the  front  edge  of  the  region 
dips  into  almost  the  whole  length  of  the  superior  precentral 
sulcus.  It  is  not  the  extent  of  the  motor  area  which  appears 
to  be  variable,  the  variant  is  the  sulcus  itself.  The  great  variety 
of  individual  pattern  exhibited  by  the  convolutions  and  sulci 
in  these  richly  convoluted  brains  gives  opportunity  for  studying 
critically  the  claim  of  value  of  these  fissures  as  landmarks  in 
the  topography  of  the  cortex.  From  this  point  of  view  their 
use  for  strict  localization  is  small.  Not  only  are  the  extremes 
of  pattern  exhibited  by  the  convolutions  extraordinarily  dif- 
ferent one  from  another,  but  the  frequency  of  the  individual 
variation  is  so  great  that  hardly  a  pair  can  be  found  in  which 
the  existent  convolutions  are,  when  compared  with  the  mi- 
nuteness applicable  to  functional  centres,  really  closely  alike. 
Schafer,  in  his  important  contribution  to  the  physiology  of 
the  motor  cortex  in  1887,^^^*  pointed  out  that  the  fissures  of  the 
cortex  do  not  mark  in  any  sense  the  boundaries  of  the  functional 
areas  of  the  organ.  Our  examination  of  the  anthropoid  brains 
we  have  worked  through  convinces  us  that  not  only  do  the 
fissures  of  the  frontal  region  not  mark  physiological  boundaries^ 
but  that  they  are  not  closely  reliable  even  as  landmarks  for 
the  functional  topography.  Their  relation  is  too  inconstant,  v. 
Monakow^*^  has  found  the  same  uncertainty  in  the  calcarine 
fissure  in  respect  to  visual  cortex.  The  degree  to  which  these 
fissures  are  subject  to  individual  variation  and  the  frequency  of 
their  asymmetry  in  the  two  hemispheres  stands  in  contrast  with 
the  constancy  from  individual  to  individual  and  greater  bilateral 
symmetry  which  holds  good  for  the  arrangement  of  the  func- 

18 


274    REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX      [Lect. 


Abdomen 


Shotiiefer 


4  thumi 


Eudid   / 


ClGsure 

Openind 


cords. 


SuUOscentrdliS. 
Mdsefcacion 


Figure  72  (from  Grunbauni  and  Sherrington). — Brain  of  a  chimpanzee  {Troglodytes 
niger).  Left  hemisphere  viewed  from  side  and  above  so  as  to  obtain  as  far  as  possible  the 
configuration  of  the  sulcus  centralis  area.  The  figure  involves,  nevertheless,  considerable 
foreshortening  about  the  top  and  bottom  of  sulcus  centralis.  The  extent  of  the  "  motor  " 
area  on  the  free  surface  of  the  hemisphere  is  indicated  by  the  black  stippling,  which 
extends  back  to  ^t  sulcus  centralis.  Much  of  the  "motor"  area  is  hidden  in  sulci; 
for  instance,  the  area  extends  into  the  sulc.  centralis  and  the  sulc.  precentrales^ 
also  into  occasional  sulci  which  cross  the  precentral  gyrus.  The  names  printed  large  on 
the  stippled  area  indicate  the  main  regions  of  the  "motor  "  area;  the  names  printed  small 
outside  the  brain,  indicate  broadly  by  their  pointing  lines  the  relative  topography  of 
some  of  the  chief  subdivisions  of  the  main  regions  of  the  "motor"  cortex.  But  there 
exists  much  overlapping  of  the  areas  and  of  their  subdivisions  which  the  diagram  does 
not  attempt  to  indicate. 

The  shaded  regions,  marked  "  EYES,"  indicate  in  the  frontal  and  occipital  regions 
respectively  the  portions  of  cortex  which,  under  faradization,  yield  conjugate  movements 
of  the  eyeballs.  But  it  is  questionable  whether  these  reactions  sufficiently  resemble  those 
of  the  "motor"  area  to  be  included  with  them.  They  are  therefore  marked  in  vertical 
shading  instead  of  stippling,  as  is  the  "motor"  area.  S.  F.  =  superior  frontal  sulcus. 
S.  Pr.  =  superior  precentral  sulcus.     I.  Pr.  =  inferior  precentral  sulcus. 


tional  centres.  A  practical  outcome  of  this  is  that  it  is  essential 
for  accurately  detailed  localization,  when  the  opening  through  the 
skull  is  of  moderate  size,  not  to  trust  to  the  anatomical  details  of 


VIII]       TOPOGRAPHY   IN  THE   CHIMPANZEE  275 


SvlccaHoso 
SulcparteCo 


SuU.  Central,      ^^^f  *  ^^^S'""^ 

Sulc.precentr.tnarg. 


Sulccalcarin 


C.S.S.  dd. 


Figure  y^  (from  Griinbaum  and  Sherrington).  —Brain  of  a  chimpanzee  {Troglodytes 
niger).  Left  hemisphere;  mesial  surface.  The  extent  of  the  "motor"  area  on  the 
free  surface  of  the  hemisphere  is  indicated  by  the  black  stippling.  On  the  stippled  area, 
"  LEG  "  indicates  that  movements  of  the  lower  limb  are  directly  represented  in  all  the 
regions  of  the  "  motor  "  area  visible  from  this  aspect.  Such  mutual  overlapping  of  the 
minuter  sub-divisions  exists  in  this  area  that  the  diagram  does  not  attempt  to  exhibit 
them.  The  pointing  line  from  "  Anus,  etc.,"  indicates  broadly  the  position  of  the  area 
whence  perineal  movements  are  primarily  elicitable. 

Sulc.  central.  =  central  fissure.  Sulc.  calcarin.  =  calcarine  fissure.  Su/c.  /arieto 
occip.  —  parieto-occipital  fissure.  Sulc.  calloso  marg.  =  calloso-marginal  fissure.  Sulc. 
precentr.  marg.  =  pre-central  fissure. 

The  single  italic  letters  mark  spots  whence,  occasionally  and  irregularly,  movements  of 
the  foot  and  leg  (//),  of  the  shoulder  and  chest  (j),  and  of  the  thumb  and  fingers  (A) 
have  been  evoked  by  strong  faradization.  Similarly  the  shaded  area  marked  "  EYES  " 
indicates  a  field  of  free  surface  of  cortex  which  under  faradization  yields  conjugate  move- 
ments of  the  eyeballs.  The  conditions  of  obtaimnent  of  these  reactions  separates  them 
from  those  characterizing  the  "motor"  area. 

the  exposed  cerebral  surface,  but  to  obtain  orientation  in  the  topo- 
graphy by  application  of  the  electrodes  and  observation  of  the 
movement,  if  any,  which  is  excited.  In  our  early  experiments 
we  thought  to  obtain  much  help  by  having  at  hand  a  brain 
of  the  same  species  already  experimented  upon  and  thought  to 
save  time  in  recording  the  results  of  the  fresh  experiment  upon 
chart   outlines   prepared   from   the   specimen   already   worked 


2^6      REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX    [Lect.  ': 

upon.     But  the  variation  of  the  convolutions  from  individual  | 

to  individual  has  been  too  great  to  allow  of  these  expedients.  | 

As  an  exception  to  the  above  general  rule  two  landmarks  of  \ 

relative    constancy  are  the  genua  of  the   sulcus  centralis,  the  \ 

Rolandic    fissure.     In  the  chimpanzee  and   gorilla   the   genua  \ 

are  two  (Fig.  72) ;  the  upper,  opposite  the  junction  between  i 

leg  area  and  arm  area,  may  be  termed  the  cruro-brachial ;  the  : 

lower,  between  arm    area   and  face  area,  may  be   termed  the  ! 

brachio-facial.     In  the  orang  there  is  in  addition  a  third  genu,  ■ 

which  from  its  relation  to  the  functional  topography  may  be  \ 

called  the  labio-linguaL     In  the  orang  the  facial  area  of  the  ? 

cortex  is  considerably  longer  from  above  down  than  in   the  i 
chimpanzee  or  gorilla. 

It  is  a  general  belief  that  for  excitation  of  the  cortex  in  man  \ 

there  is  needed  an  intensity  of  faradism  much  greater  than  that  \ 

sufficing  for  the  cortex  of  the  monkey.     Actually  comparing  the  ' 
excitability  of  the  cortex  of  the  anthropoid  with  that  of  the 

bonnet  monkey  by  employing  exactly  the  same  current  in  each  '■ 

case,  we  found  the  excitability  as  measured  by  the  least  intensity  \ 

of  current  required   to   evoke    motor  reaction  practically  the  ' 

same  in  the  anthropoid  and  in  the  lower  ape.^^     The  motor  \ 

cortex  of  the  anthropoid,  though  undoubtedly  far  more  complex  ~\ 

in  many  ways  than  that  of  the  lower  ape,  remains  as  readily  ; 

amenable  to  electric  stimulation.     Cushing  of  Baltimore   and  \ 

Krause  of  Berlin    find   this   holds   good   also  for   the   human  \ 

brain,  and  that  it  is  not  necessary  to  employ  strong  faradization.  \ 

In  the  majority  of  the  anthropoids  upon  which  we  have  experi-  \ 

mented  cortical  epilepsy  has  been  quite  easily  provoked,  just  as  ^ 

it  is  in  the  small  monkeys.  \ 

In  the  precentral  gyrus,  the  sequence  of  representation  of  1 

the  musculature  starting  from  below  upward  follows  broadly  \ 

that  known  for  the  lower  apes.     The  sequence  runs  —  tongue,  1 

jaw,  mouth,  nose,  ear,  eyelids,  neck,  hand,  wrist,  elbow,  shoulder,  \ 

chest,  abdomen,  hip,  knee,  ankle,  toes,  and  perineal  muscles.  ; 

It  is  noticeable  that  movements  of  the  eyeballs  do  not  occur  \ 

in  this  list     As  did  Beevor  and  Horsley  in  their  orang,^^?  so  j 

we   in  the  chimpanzee,  gorilla,  and  orang  find  a  frontal  area  \ 


VIII]  FACILITY   OF   EXCITATION  277 

extending  into  the  middle  and  inferior  frontal  convolutions 
excitation  of  which  gives  conjugate  deviation  of  the  eyeballs 
to  the  opposite  side.  We  find  this  area  separated  from  the 
area  yielding  the  other  movements  by  an  intervening  space. 
We  find  this  intervening  space  broken,  however,  by  small  areas 
whence  movements  of  the  eyeballs  can  be  elicited,  partially 
bridging  between  it  and  the  upper  facial  region  on  the 
precentral  convolution. 

The  sequence  of  representation  of  movement  which  we  note 
follows  a  plan  more  in  accordance  with  the  order  of  the  spinal 
series  of  segments  than  that  hitherto  obtained.  Between  the 
place  of  representation  of  shoulder  and  that  of  hip  is  an  area 
which,  next  to  the  shoulder,  yields  unilateral  movement  of  the 
chest  muscles,  and,  next  to  the  hip,  yields  unilateral  movement 
of  the  abdominal  muscles,  and  furthest  upward  lies  a  focus  for 
perineal  muscles. 

In  our  experience,  in  accord  with  the  original  observations 
by  Hitzig^  on  the  lower  apes,  the  electrodes  when  placed  upon 
the  surface  of  the  post-central  convolution  fail  to  evoke  any 
obvious  effect,  though  when  placed  with  an  even  weaker  current 
upon  the  precentral  they  evoke  the  regular  reaction.  In  our 
experience,  though  small  lesions  in  the  precentral  convolution 
caused  marked  paralyses  and  descending  spinal  degenerations, 
similar  and  larger  lesions  in  the  post-central  did  not  produce 
even  temporary  paralysis  nor  any  unequivocal  degeneration. 

With  regard  to  the  degenerations,  it  is  noteworthy  that  from 
a  hand-area  lesion  the  spinal  pyramidal  degeneration  shows  in 
some  chimpanzees  a  ventral  direct  pyramidal  tract  of  size  not 
obviously  inferior  to  that  of  a  man.  But  this  ventral  direct 
tract  does  not  appear  to  be  present  in  all  individual  chim- 
panzees,—  a  fact  agreeing  with  Flechsig's^^  discovery  of  its 
variability  in  man.  The  hand-area  lesion  gives  a  heavy  degen- 
eration of  the  homolateral  pyramidal  tract  in  the  lateral  column 
on  the  same  side  as  the  cerebral  lesion. 

As  regards  symptoms  resulting  from  the  cortical  lesions, 
extirpation  of  a  great  part  if  not  of  the  whole  of  the  hand  area 
from  the  right  hemisphere  caused  an  immediate  severe  crossed 


2/8     REACTIONS   OF   THE   MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect. 

brachioplegia  without  the  slightest  sign  of  paresis  in  either  face 
or  leg.  The  paresis  affected  the  fingers  most ;  these  were  kept 
helplessly  semi-extended,  the  wrist  being  dropped.  The  elbow 
seemed  little  if  at  all  affected,  but  the  shoulder  seemed  dis- 
tinctly paretic,  there  being  difficulty  in  raising  or  abducting  the 
upper  arm.  The  paresis  diminished  quite  rapidly,  and  in  six 
weeks'  time  the  animal  had  in  large  measure  recovered  the 
usefulness  of  the  limb. 

A  lesion  in  the  leg-area  similarly  caused  temporary  paresis 
of  the  opposite  leg,  especially  in  the  toes  and  at  the  ankle-joint. 
The  lesion  was  smaller  and  the  recovery  more  rapid  than  with 
the  arm-area  lesion.  The  knee-jerk,  which  showed  no  alteration 
under  the  arm-area  lesion,  here  showed  exaltation  immediately, 
that  is,  a  quarter  of  an  hour  after  the  leg-area  lesion.  Weeks 
later,  when  the  paresis  to  inspection  had  passed  off,  the  knee- 
jerk  still  exhibited  greater  briskness  on  the  crossed  side. 

We  have  seen  often  confirmed  what  our  predecessors  '^^'^  with 
the  orang  have  well  pointed  out,  namely,  the  greater  integration 
of  localized  representation  of  movements  in  the  anthropoid  as 
compared  with  the  lower  ape.  There  is  not  one  of  the  fingers 
that  we  have  not  seen  move  separately  and  alone  under  excita- 
tion of  certain  points  of  the  cortex ;  again,  isolated  movement  of 
the  pinna  of  the  ear,  of  the  tip  of  the  tongue,  rare  in  the  lower 
monkeys,  are  easily  obtainable  in  the  anthropoid. 

As  to  the  extent  of  the  so-called  motor  area,  from  our  obser- 
vations we  think  it  probable  that  in  the  anthropoid  brain  as 
much  of  that  area  lies  hidden  from  the  surface  in  the  sulci  as 
is  actually  exposed  on  the  free  surface  on  the  convolutions. 
Nevertheless  we  indorse  the  opinion  expressed  by  Beevor  and 
Horsley  that  the  so-called  motor  area  in  the  anthropoid  brain 
forms  a  smaller  fraction  of  the  total  surface  than  it  does  in  the 
lower  types  of  monkey.  If  it  has  grown  in  extent  —  as  undoubt- 
edly it  seems  to  have  done  —  other  regions  belonging  to  those 
so-called  "silent"  fields  whence  electric  stimulation  excites  no 
obvious  response  have  increased  still  more.  It  is  especially 
with  the  exploration  of  that  great  inexcitable  field  that  research 
has  to  deal.    The  discoveries  of  Flechsig,  v.  Monakow,  Dejerine, 


VIII]  EXTENT  OF   MOTOR  CORTEX  279 

Mott,  Campbell,  the  Vogts,  and  others  yearly  advance  further 
in  the  problem. 

The  results  on  the  gorilla  ^25  confirm  those  we  have  obtained 
on  the  chimpanzee,  though  in  the  gorilla  and  orang  in  my 
experience  eyeball  movements  have  been  elicited  from  a  larger 
field  of  the  frontal  cortex  than  in  the  chimpanzee,  an  upper 
area  yielding  eye  movement  on  a  level  with  the  hand  area  being 
more  readily  discoverable. 

That  the  free  surface  of  the  post-central  convolution  belongs 
to  the  motor  cortex  we  have  not  found.  Brodman^^*  and 
Campbell  ^"^*  have  since  called  attention  to  marked  structural 
differences  between  the  cortex  respectively  behind  and  in  front 
of  the  central  sulcus.  The  arrangement  of  the  fibres  and  the 
character  of  the  cells  is  different.  Ramon-y-Cajal,^^*  using  the 
Golgi  method,  and  Flechsig^^ia  following  the  myelinization,  had 
also  previously  drawn  distinction  between  the  structure  of  the 
two  convolutions  divided  by  this  great  fissure ;  and  observations 
by  Mott,  A.  Tschermak,  and  others  had  indicated  an  especially 
close  connection  of  the  post-central  gyrus  with  ascending  pre- 
sumably afferent  paths.  Evidence  of  this  last  by  excitation 
methods  is  of  course  difficult  to  obtain,  but  nothing  in  our 
experiments  is  contrary  to  it,  and  some  occasional  results  that 
have  come  before  me  in  experimenting  by  excitation  lend  them- 
selves to  such  an  explanation.  To  enter  upon  these  here  would 
lead  too  far  from  our  main  interest  now. 

It  is  natural  to  inquire  whether  reciprocal  innervation  is 
exemplified  by  reactions  from  the  cortex.  That  inhibition  of 
muscular  contraction  is  obtainable  by  artificial  excitation  of  the 
cortex  was  early  noted  by  Bubnoff  and  Heidenhain  ^  in  the  dog 
and  by  Exner  ^^  in  the  rabbit.  I  have  myself  worked  chiefly 
with  the  monkey .^^^'  ^^*  In  that  animal  the  ocular  axes  are  par- 
allel, and  the  setting  of  the  eyeball  in  the  orbit  is  such  that 
the  tensions  of  its  connections,  apart  from  unequal  activity  of 
its  extrinsic  muscles,  are  in  equilibrium  when  the  globes  are 
approximately  parallel.  That  is  their  primary  position.  This 
can  be  shown  in  various  ways.  Thus,  if  the  III,  IV,  and  VI 
nerves  are  all  severed  the  eyeballs  assume  this  primary  position. 


280      REACTIONS   OF   THE   MOTOR   CORTEX    [Lect. 

If  one  eyeball  be  then  rotated  by  the  finger  or  fixation  forceps 
to  right  or  left  or  up  and  down  a  considerable  resistance  is  felt, 
and  on  letting  it  go  the  globus  springs  back  at  once  to  the 
primary  position.  So  also  in  chloroformization.  In  the  early 
stage  of  chloroformization  the  eyes  enter  various  positions  of 
squint  —  in  the  monkey  a  very  usual  position  is  bilateral  diver- 
gence upward  and  outward.  When  the  narcosis  has  become 
profound,  the  eyes  revert  to  approximate  paralleHsm  in  the 
primary  position.  On  being  then  displaced  by  the  finger  they 
at  once  swing  into  the  primary  median  position  again.  So  also 
immediately  after  death,  before  rigor  mortis  has  set  in. 

If  III  and  IV  cranial  nerves  of  one  side,  e,  g.  left,  have  been 
severed,  so  that  rectus  externus  remains  the  only  unparalyzed 
ocular  muscle,  appropriate  excitation  of  the  cortex  cerebri  pro- 
duces conjugate  movement  of  both  eyes  towards  the  opposite 
side,  /.  e.  from  left  toward  right,  the  left  eye  travelling  however 
only  so  far  as  the  median  line.  Inhibition  of  the  tonus  and  of 
the  active  contraction  of  rectus  externus  can  thus  be  elicited 
from  the  cortex.  The  reaction  is  obtainable  from  all  that  por- 
tion of  the  cortex  which  on  excitation  gives  conjugate  lateral 
deviation  of  the  eyes,  i.  e,  from  the  area  discovered  by  Ferrier  '^ 
in  the  frontal  region,  and  from  that  discovered  by  Schafer  ^^"*  in 
the  occipital  region. 

This  inhibition  is  obtainable  from  the  frontal  area  after  com- 
plete removal  of  the  occipital  lobe.  It  is  conversely  obtainable 
from  the  occipital  area  after  complete  removal  of  the  frontal  area. 
After  a  deep  frontal  section  across  the  hemisphere  and  into  the 
lateral  ventricle  (partly  entering  the  internal  capsule)  so  as  to 
sever  occipital  from  frontal  cortex  in  the  manner  practised  by 
Munk  and  Obregia,^28  ^he  reaction  is  obtainable  undiminished 
from  both  the  frontal  and  from  the  occipital  areas  separately. 

The  cortex  is  not  essential  to  the  reaction.  It  is  obtain- 
able from  the  corona  radiata  underlying  the  frontal  cortex  after 
complete  ablation  of  the  frontal  cortex  itself  It  is  obtainable 
from  the  corona  radiata  running  downwards  and  forwards  from 
the  occipital  cortex  after  free  removal  of  the  latter.  It  is  obtain- 
able by  direct  excitation  of  the  internal  capsule  itself     From 


VIII]         RECIPROCAL   INNERVATION   FROM  281 

the  internal  capsule  it  is  elicitable  at  two  distinct  places,  one  in 
front  of  the  other  behind  the  genu  of  the  capsule.  It  is  obtain- 
able by  excitation  of  the  cross-section  of  corpus  callosunt  about 
3-5  millimeters  behind  the  genu;  also  from  corpus  callosunt 
at  the  splenium.  The  section  was  laid  bare  as  in  Mott  and 
Schafer's  ^2^*  second  method.  The  reaction  as  obtained  from 
corpus  callosunt  has  in  my  hands  proved  comparatively  irregu- 
lar. It  is  evident  that  the  action  of  arrest  may  take  place,  in 
centres  which  are  subcortical. 

E.  H.  Hering  and  myself^'^  made  observations  on  limb 
movements  elicited  from  the  cortex  and  found  evidence  of  a 
similar  co-ordination  in  regard  to  them.  As  in  the  experi- 
ments of  Bubnoff  and  Heidenhain,*''^  the  degree  of  narcotiza- 
tion formed  an  important  condition  for  the  observations.  The 
narcosis  must  not  be  too  profound.  It  is  best,  starting  with 
the  animal  in  a  condition  of  deep  etherization,  to  allow  that 
condition  gradually  to  diminish.  As  this  is  done  it  almost 
constantly  happens  that  at  a  certain  stage  of  anaesthesia  the 
limbs,  instead  of  hanging  slack  and  flaccid,  assume  and  maintain 
a  position  of  flexion  at  certain  joints,  notably  at  elbow  and  hip. 
This  condition  of  tonic  contraction  having  been  assumed,  the 
narcosis  is  as  far  as  possible  kept  at  that  particular  grade  of 
intensity.  The  area  of  cortex  cerebri  previously  ascertained 
to  produce  under  faradization  extension  of  the  elbow-joint  or 
hip-joint  is  then  excited. 

For  clearness  of  description  let  us  suppose  the  left  hemi- 
sphere excited,  and  the  limb  affected  the  right.  The  result 
of  excitation  of  the  appropriate  focus  in  the  cortex,  e.  g, 
that  presiding  over  extension  of  the  elbow,  is  an  immediate 
relaxation  of  the  biceps  with  active  contraction  of  the  tri- 
ceps. As  regards  the  condition  of  the  biceps,  the  relaxa- 
tion is  usually  so  striking  that  merely  to  place  the  finger  on  it 
is  enough  to  convince  the  observer  that  the  muscle  relaxes. 
The  following  is  however  a  good  mode  of  studying  the  phe- 
nomenon: in  a  monkey  with  strongly  developed  musculature 
the  fore  arm,  maintained  by  the  above-mentioned  steady  tonic 
flexion  at  an  angle  with  the  upper  arm  of  somewhat  less  than 


282      REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX     [Lect. 

90°  is  lightly  supported  by  the  one  hand  of  the  observer, 
while  with  the  finger  and  thumb  of  the  other  the  belly  of  the 
contracted  biceps  is  felt  through  the  skin.  On  exciting  the 
cortex  the  contracted  mass  becomes  suddenly  soft,  melting 
under  the  observer's  touch.  At  the  same  time  the  observer's 
hand  supporting  the  animal's  fore  arm  tends  to  be  pushed  down 
with  a  force  unmistakably  greater  than  that  which  the  mere 
weight  of  the  limb  would  exert.  If  the  triceps  itself  be  felt  at 
this  time,  it  is  easy  to  perceive  that  it  enters  contraction,  becom- 
ing increasingly  hard  and  tense,  even  when  its  points  of  attach- 
ment are  allowed  to  approximate,  and  the  passive  tensile 
strain  in  it  should  lessen.  If  the  limb  be  left  unsupported  the 
movement  is  one  of  simple  extension  at  the  elbow-joint.  On 
discontinuing  the  excitation  of  the  cortex  the  fore  arm  usually 
immediately,  or  almost  immediately,  returns  to  its  previous 
posture  of  flexion,  which  is  again  as  before  steadily  maintained. 
Conversely,  when,  as  not  unfrequently  occurs  in  conditions 
of  narcosis  resembling  that  above  referred  to,  the  arm  has 
assumed  a  posture  of  extension  and  this  is  tonic  and  maintained, 
the  opportunity  may  be  taken  to  excite  the  appropriate  focus 
in  the  cortex  for  flexion  of  fore  arm  or  upper  arm.  Triceps 
is  then  found  to  relax,  and  biceps  at  the  same  time  to  enter 
into  active  contraction.  If  the  biceps  be  hindered  from  actually 
moving  the  arm,  the  prominence  at  the  back  of  the  upper  arm 
due  to  the  contracted  triceps  is  seen  simply  to  sink  down  and 
become  flattened.  When  examined  by  palpation  the  muscle  is 
felt  to  become  more  or  less  suddenly  soft,  and  the  biceps  at  the 
same  time  to  become  more  tense  than  before.  The  move- 
ment of  the  limb,  when  allowed  to  proceed  unhindered,  is 
flexion  with  some  supination.  It  is  noteworthy  that  in  this 
experiment  not  every  part  of  the  large  triceps  mass  becomes 
relaxed ;  a  part  of  the  muscle  which  extends  from  the  humerus 
to  the  scapula  does  not  in  this  experiment  relax  with  the  rest 
of  the  muscle.  This  part,  if  the  scapula  be  fixed,  acts  as  a 
retractor  of  the  upper  arm,  and  is  not  necessarily  an  antag- 
onist of  the  flexors  of  the  elbow.  This  part  of  the  triceps  we 
observed  sometimes  enter  active  contraction  at  the  same  time 


VIII]  INHIBITION   FROM   THE   CORTEX  283 

as  the  flexors  of  the  elbow.  Under  use  of  currents  of  moderate 
intensity  we  found  that  not  from  one  and  the  same  spot  in  the 
cortex  can  relaxation  and  contraction  of  a  given  muscle  be 
evoked  at  different  times,  but  that  the  two  effects  are  provocable 
at  different,  sometimes  widely  separate,  points  of  the  cortex, 
and  are  there  found  regularly. 

We  obtained  analogous  results  in  the  muscles  acting  at  the 
hip-joint  In  the  narcotized  animal  the  hip-joint  is  being  main- 
tained in  flexion,  the  thighs  being  drawn  up  on  the  trunk, 
excitation  of  the  region  of  the  cortex  previously  ascertained, 
when  the  limbs  hang  slack,  to  evoke  extension  of  the  hip,  pro- 
duces relaxation  of  the  flexors  of  the  hip  and  at  the  same  time 
active  contraction  of  the  extensors  of  the  thigh.  We  examined 
particularly  the  psoas-iliacuSy  and  the  tensor  fascice  femorisy 
also  the  short  and  long  adductor  muscles.  Each  of  these  was 
found  to  relax  under  appropriate  cortical  excitation.  If  the 
knee  were  held  by  the  observer  it  was  found  at  the  time  of 
relaxation  of  the  flexors  of  the  hip  to  be  forced  downward  by 
active  extension  of  the  hip. 

Similarly  with  other  groups  of  antagonistic  muscles,  both 
those  of  the  small  apical  joints  of  the  limb,  e.  g»  flexors  and 
extensors  of,  the  digits,  and  those  of  the  large  proximal  joints, 
e.g.  adductors  and  abductors  of  the  shoulder.  At  these  also 
instances  of  reciprocal  innervation  were  obtained.  By  antagonis- 
tic muscles  I  mean  only  what  are  termed  true  antagonistics ;  I  do 
not  include  the  cases  where  one  muscle  fixes  a  joint  enabling 
another  muscle  to  thus  act  better  on  another  joint  —  H.  E.  Her- 
ing's  pseudoantagonists.^^^  Hering  has  carefully  analyzed  ^^^ 
the  co-ordination  of  such  pseudoantagonists  in  the  action  of 
clenching  the  fist.  He  has  shown  that  when  that  movement  is 
evoked  in  the  monkey  by  excitation  of  the  cortex  cerebri  the  ex- 
tensors of  the  wrist  are  thrown  into  action  simultaneously  with 
the  long  flexor  of  the  fingers.  But  there  was  no  evidence 
that  the  true  antagonists  were  ever  thrown  into  simultaneous 
activity. 

That  a  part  of  the  triceps  brachii  (that  retracting  the  upper 
arm)  should  actively  contract  exactly  when  another  part  (that  ex- 


284      REACTIONS   OF  THE    MOTOR   CORTEX    [Lect.  ] 

tending  the  elbow)  becomes  relaxed  is  exactly  comparable  with  1 

a  phenomenon  which  can  be  noted  in  the  limb  under  spinal  re-  ■ 

flexes,  both  in  triceps  itself  and  in  quadriceps  femoris.    Beevor  and  j 

others  have  shown  that  different  parts  of  what  in  gross  anatomy  \ 

is  denominated  one  single  muscle  are  used  separately  in  various  i 

movements.     And  Hering  and  I  similarly  saw  in  the  quadriceps  1 

femoris,  on  exciting  the  cortical  region  yielding  extension  of  the  j 

hip,  a  relaxation  of  a  part  of  the  quadriceps  (a  part  which  flexes  j 

the  hip)  with  contraction  of  another  part  (which  extends  the  1 

knee).     I  have  also  noted  that  in  the  monkey  by  stimulating  i 
the  appropriate  cortical  area  for  flexion  of  the  knee  the  knee- 
jerk  is  temporarily  depressed  or  suppressed  completely. 

The  results  obtained  from  the  internal  capsule  were  as  strik-  i 

ing  as  those  obtained  from  the  cortex  itself.    From  separate  points  | 

of  the  cross-section  of  the  capsula,  relaxation  of  various  muscles  i 

was  evoked.     Among  the  muscles  whose  inhibition  was  directly  i 

observed  were  supitiator  longus  and  biceps  brachii,  the  triceps,  the  j 

deltoid^  the  extensor  cruris^  the   hamstring   group,    the    flexor  \ 
muscles  of  the  ankle-joint,  and  the  sternomastoid. 

The  spots  in  the  cross-section  of  the  capsula  which  yielded  j 
the  inhibitions  were  constant,  that  is,  the  position  of  each  when  I 
observed  remained  constant  throughout  the  experiment.  The  \ 
area  of  the  capsular  cross-section  at  which  the  inhibition  of  the  i 
activity  of,  e,  g.  the  triceps,  muscle  can  be  evoked  is  separate  I 
from  (that  is  to  say  not  the  same  as)  that  area  whence  excitation  j 
evokes  contraction  of  the  triceps  (or  of  that  part  of  the  triceps  ! 
inhibition  of  which  is  now  referred  to).  On  the  other  hand,  the  i 
area  of  the  section  of  the  internal  capsule,  whence  inhibition  of  1 
the  muscle  is  elicited,  corresponds  with  the  area  whence  con- 
traction of  its  antagonistic  muscles  can  be  evoked.  Yet  synchro-  \ 
nous  contraction  of  such  pairs  of  muscles  as  gastrocnemius  and  i 
peroneus  longus  is  obtainable  from  the  cortex.  The  observa-  \ 
tions  make  it  clear  that  "  reciprocal  imiervation  "  in  antagonistic  ! 
muscles  is  obtainable  by  excitation  of  the  fibres  of  the  internal  \ 
capsule.  Topolanski  ^^^  observed  them  on  exciting  the  corpora  \ 
quadrigemina  (rabbit).  It  is  probable  therefore  that  the  in-  ] 
hibition  elicitable  from  the  cortex  cerebri  is  not  in  these  cases  j 


VIII]     INHIBITION   PROBABLY  SUBCORTICAL        285 

chiefly  or  at  all  due  to  an  interaction  of  cortical  neurones  one 
with  another. 

Exner  drew  from  his  observations  on  the  rabbit  a  similar  in- 
ference that  the  inhibitory  phenomena  had  their  chief  seat  in  the 
spinal  mechanisms  though  elicited  from  the  cortex.  My  own 
inference  has  been  that  the  seat  of  inhibition  in  these  reactions 
from  the  "  motor  "  cortex  Hes  probably  at  the  place  of  confluence 
of  conducting  channels  in  a  common  path,  likely  enough  at  their 
confluence  upon  the  '^  final  common  path,''  the  motor  neurone, 
that  is  at  the  ultimate  synapse.  But  it  may  well  be,  indeed  is  in 
the  highest  degree  Hkely,  that  in  other  fields  of  action  one  corti- 
cal element  inhibits  another  cortical  element. 

In  '^willed''  movements  of  the  eyeballs  of  the  monkey  the  same 
kind  of  co-ordination  was  revealed  in  some  observations  I  ob- 
tained on  this  point.  When  the  III  and  IV  cranial  nerves  had 
been  resected  intracranially  and  the  animals  in  ten  days  or  so 
had  recovered  completely  from  the  surgical  interference,  the  eye 
movements  were  examined. 

In  these  animals  if  the  gaze  was  attracted  to  an  object,  e.  g. 
food,  held  level  with  the  eyes  and  to  the  right  of  the  median 
plane  (left  III  and  IV  nerves  cut)  the  left  eye  looked  straight 
forward,  the  right  looked  to  the  right.  If  the  object  was  then 
shifted  more  to  the  right  or  less  to  the  right,  the  right  eye  fol- 
lowed it,  moving  as  the  object  was  moved,  while  the  left  eye 
remained  motionless,  looking  straight  forward  all  the  time. 
But  when  the  object  was  held  to  the  left  of  the  median  plane 
both  eyes  were  directed  upon  it,  apparently  quite  accurately, 
When  the  object  was  shifted  farther  and  farther  to  the  left  both 
eyes  followed  it  with  a  steady  conjugate  movement  not  detectably 
different  from  the  normal.  When  the  object  was  carried  from 
the  left-hand  verge  of  the  field  back  toward  the  median  plane 
both  eyes  followed  it  as  accurately  as  before.  If  the  object  was 
moved  suddenly  from  the  extreme  left-hand  edge  of  the  field  up 
to  the  median  plane  both  eyes  immediately  and  apparently 
equally  quickly  reverted  to  parallelism  with  that  plane.  Or,  if 
the  object  were  suddenly  brought  back  from  the  left  edge  of 
the  visual  field  to  some  point  intermediate  between  that  and  the 


286      REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX    [Lect. 

median  plane  both  eyes  at  once  shifted  apparently  equally  to  a 
correspondingly  diminished  deviation  from  the  primary  posi- 
tion. These  actions  must  mean  that  in  the  left  eye  relaxation  of 
rectus  externus  kept  accurate  time  and  step  with  contraction  of 
rectus  externus  of  right  eye.  And  the  action  of  the  left  rectus 
externus  gives  presumably  a  faithful  picture  of  a  synchronous 
process  going  forward  in  the  right  rectus  internus. 

It  is  interesting  to  recall  that  in  the  seventeenth  century 
Descartes  in  his  De   Homine,^  discussing   willed   movements, 

B 


Figure  74.  —  A.  Figure  from  the  De  Nomine  of  Descartes,  edit,  of  1662^  in  which  he  illus- 
trates his  conception  of  the  co-ordination  of  the  antagonistic  muscles  of  the  eyeball  by  the 
above  drawing  from  his  own  hand. 

B.    Figure  illustrating  the  same  text  in   De   Homine,  edit,   of   1677*;  in  this  the 
sketch  by  Descartes  has  been  much  elaborated. 


suggested  for  the  mechanism  of  the  lateral  movements  of  the 
eyeball,  with  which  he  deals  in  some  detail,  a  co-ordination 
much  resembling  reciprocal  innervation.  He  urged  that  the 
vital  spirits  were  conducted  into  the  external  rectus  by  valved 
channels  in  that  muscle,  and  at  the  same  time  were  from  the 
internal  rectus  led  out  by  valved  channels,  so  that  as  the  one 


VIII]  RECIPROCAL  INHIBITION  287 

muscle  became  tense  by  distension  the  other  became  flaccid  by 
emptying.  He  furnished  with  his  own  pencil  a  figure  illus- 
trating the  mechanism  as  he  conceived  it  (Fig.  74).  There  is 
an  essential  resemblance  between  his  scheme  and  that  of  '*  recip- 
rocal innervation,"  except  that  he  imagined  the  mechanism  a 
peripheral  one,  that  is  to  say  that  the  "  inhibition,"  as  we  now 
term  it,  had  its  seat  in  the  muscle,  not  in  the  nerve-centres 
themselves. 

Again,  early  last  century  (1823)  Charles  Bell,  in  a  footnote  to 
a  paper  in  the  Philosophical  Transactions,^^  argued  a  similar  kind 
of  co-ordinate  mechanism  in  the  execution  of  willed  movements. 
He  wrote :  "  The  nerves  have  been  considered  so  generally  as 
instruments  for  stimulating  the  muscles,  without  thought  of  their 
acting  in  the  opposite  capacity,  that  some  additional  illustration 
may  be  necessary  here.  Through  the  nerves  is  established  the 
connection  between  the  muscles,  not  only  that  connection  by 
which  muscles  combine  to  one  effort  but  also  that  relation  be- 
tween the  classes  of  muscles  by  which  the  one  relaxes  and  the 
other  contracts.  I  appended  a  weight  to  a  tendon  of  an  ex- 
tensor muscle  which  gently  stretched  it  and  drew  out  the  muscle ; 
and  I  found  that  the  contraction  of  the  opponent  flexor  was 
attended  with  a  descent  of  the  weight,  which  indicated  the  relaxa- 
tion of  the  extensor."  "  If  such  a  relationship  be  established, 
through  the  distribution  of  the  nerves,  between  the  muscles  of 
the  eyelids  and  the  superior  oblique  muscles  of  the  eyeball,  the 
one  will  relax  while  the  other  contracts."  But  like  Descartes  he 
pictured  a  peripheral  inhibition,  for  he  says :  "  If  we  suppose 
that  the  influence  of  the  4th  nerve  is,  on  certain  occasions,  to 
cause  a  relaxation  of  the  muscle  to  which  it  goes,  the  eyeball 
must  be  then  rolled  upwards."  Descartes  and  Bell,  therefore, 
with  remarkable  prescience  imagined  the  existence  of  an  action 
of  nerve  on  muscle  just  such  as  was  later  actually  discovered  by 
the  Webers^^  in  the  vagal  inhibition  of  the  heart  —  an  inhibition 
which  Volkmann  ^'*  previous  to  the  Webers  met  in  the  course  of 
experiment,  but  unevpectant  of  it,  rejected  ^^  as  illusory  and  due 
to  some  experimental  error. 


288      REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect.     i 

i 
As  for  salient  objective  differences  observable  between  move*    \ 
ments  elicited  from  the  so-called    "  motor "  cortex  and   thos#    ^ 
of  spinal  reflexes,  these  are  for  the  most  part  less  clear  than     l 
might  at  first  be  supposed.     The  general  statement  that  the  co-     i 
ordination  is  of  a  "  higher "  kind  in  the  former  has  doubtless 
truth,  but  it  proves  vague  when  details  are  demanded.     A  co- 
ordination though  simple  may  yet  be  perfect.     The  co-ordination     ; 
by  which  the  leg  is  drawn  up  in  the  spinal  **  flexion-reflex  "  seems 
as  perfect  as  when  the  limb  is  drawn  up  by  stimulation  of  the     ' 
cortex.     It  is  true  that  in  the  dog's  scratching  movement  elicited     i 
as  a  spinal  reflex  after  transection  of  the  cord  the  foot  in  my 
experience  practically  never  attains  accurately  the  site  of  the    i 
stimulation,  although  broadly  directed  toward  it.      Were  the 
movement  elicited  from  the  cortex  one  would  expect  it  to  be     ■ 
more  accurate  in  this  respect.     But  I  have  never  succeeded  in     ; 
eliciting  this  action  from  the  cortex,  and  so  am  unable  to  insti- 
tute the  comparison.  ; 

If  by  higher  co-ordination  it  be  meant  that  larger  groups  of 
reflex  systems  are  simultaneously  thrown  into  —  or  out   of —    ' 
action  under  cortical  excitation  than  in  merely  spinal  reflexes, 
that  is  more  than  probable.     Yet  we  must  admit  that  the  field 
of  musculature  thrown  into  action  by  a  focal  stimulation  of  the    ] 
cortex  seems  in  some  cases  extremely  limited.      Thus  A.  S. 
Griinbaum  and  myself  have  seen  that  in  the  chimpanzee  and 
gorilla   any   single   manual   digit  can  be  moved  isolatedly  by 
stimulation  of  the  cortex.     We  must  not  forget,  however,  that 
with  even  a  small  movement  the  field  of  inhibition  may  yet  be     \ 
wide,  for  I  have  on  occasion  noted  inhibition  of  muscles  of  the 
shoulder  when  the  thumb  was  moved  under  cortical  excitation, 
the  shoulder  previously  being  unrelaxed. 

There  is  the  well-known  clonic  after-discharge  following  | 
cortical  stimulation.  But  a  marked  after-discharge  is  also  | 
usual  in  spinal  reflexes,  rhythmic  in  rhythmic  reflexes,  tetano-  | 
clonic  in  tetanic  reflexes  e.  g,  in  the  "  flexion-reflex,"  and  is  \ 
sometimes  enormously  prolonged  (Figs.  49  and  57).  So  that  \ 
even  this  difference  is  less  marked  than  is  customarily  thought.  I 
Certain  other  differences  appear  to  me  more  significant.    When 


VIII]       CORTICAL   AND    SPINAL   REACTIONS         289 

a  spinal  reflex  is  prolonged  under  strong  stimulation  its  dis- 
charge spreads,  developing  what  Dr.  Hughlings  Jackson  ^  has 
termed  a  "  march."  The  **  march  "  of  the  spinal  reaction  tends 
to  transgress  the  mediajt  line  more  than  does  that  of  a  cortical 
reaction,  which  tends  rather  to  spread  unilaterally.  Also  the 
progress  of  the  spinal  **  march"  runs  a  more  rapid  course  than 
does  the  cortical. 

In  the  synthesis  of  movement  in  the  animal  it  is  obvious 
that  these  reactions  elicitable  from  the  motor  cortex  fall  into 
three  groups,  like  the  three  groups  above  distinguished  in 
spinal  reflexes  (Lect.  IV).  In  one  group  the  movement  evoked 
from  the  cortex  of  one  hemisphere  seems  a  fraction  of  a  natural 
movement,  the  natural  movement  requiring  in  its  completeness 
the  co-operation  of  the  symmetrical  area  of  the  cortex  of  the 
opposite  hemisphere.  Opening  of  the  jaw  as  elicited  from  one 
hemisphere,  e.  g.  the  left,  is  seen  after  the  jaw  is  split  at  the 
symphysis  to  be  executed  by  the  muscles  of  the  crossed,  i.  e. 
right,  half  of  the  jaw,  the  muscles  of  the  left  half  being  very 
slightly  activated  or  not  at  all.  This  cortical  movement  is 
evidently  incomplete  and  fractional.  The  inference  is  unavoid- 
able that  in  the  natural  undeviated  opening  of  the  mouth  the 
actions  of  symmetrical  areas  of  the  right  and  left  cortices  are 
coupled  as  ''allied''  reactions.  In  a  second  group,  instanced  by 
conjugate  lateral  deviation  of  the  eyeballs  toward  the  opposite  side, 
it  is  equally  obvious  that  the  reactions  of  symmetrical  areas  of 
the  right  and  left  cortices  are  related  one  to  another  as  **  antago- 
nistic "  reactions.  Such  reactions  have  inhibitory  relation  one 
to  another  (151,  304).  They  must  have  this  inhibitory  relation 
even  when  combined,  as  Mott  and  Schafer  showed  they  can  be, 
to  yield  convergence  of  the  ocular  axes  under  bilateral  excita- 
tion of  the  right  and  left  hemispheres.  In  a  third  group  of 
cases  the  reactions  of  symmetrical  cortical  areas  right  and  left 
seem  neutral  one  to  another.  Thus,  with  the  area  which  yields 
movement  of  the  thumb  that  reaction  seems  neither  to  reinforce 
nor  to  interfere  with  the  similar  reaction  evoked  from  the  twin 
area  of  the  opposite  hemisphere.  That  the  reactions  are  really 
wholly   neutral   one   to   another   is  of  course   difficult   to   say 

19 


290      REACTIONS   OF  THE    MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect. 

because  the  experimental  observations  are  carried  out  under 
narcosis,  and  the  narcosis  probably  sets  in  abeyance  many  co- 
ordinating mechanisms  of  the  brain  itself.  It  is  clear,  however, 
that  the  same  broad  groups  of  interrelationship  (alliance,  inter- 
ference, neutrality)  as  were  traced  in  bulbospinal  reflexes  re- 
appear also  between  motor  reactions  of  symmetrical  areas  of 
the  cortex  of  the  hemispheres. 

It  is  striking  that  the  complete,  /.  e,  perfectly  balanced, 
bilaterality  of  motor  representation  with  which  the  Broca  motor 
speech  centre  is  credited,  no  doubt  justly,  is  exceptional  in 
the  motor  cortex.  Beevor  and  Horsley  pointed  out  that  move- 
ments of  perfectly  balanced  bilaterality  are  of  much  rarer  dis- 
tribution in  the  cortex  of  the  hemisphere  than  was  generally 
supposed.  I  incline  to  think  that  even  the  small  category  of 
such  movements  which  they  admit  will  have  to  be  reduced 
further  by  the  removal  from  it  of  "  mastication."  Certain  it  is 
that  for  a  group  of  movements  to  be  perfectly  bilaterally  repre- 
sented in  an  area  of  one  hemisphere  and  not  equally  in  the 
corresponding  area  of  the  other  hemisphere,  the  state  of  things 
generally  supposed  for  the  Broca  centre,  is  an  arrangement 
wholly  unknown  in  the  motor  cortex.  It  shows  how  different 
must  be  the  operations  of  the  Broca  area  from  those  of  the 
areas  of  the  so  called  motor  cortex.  Griinbaum  and  myself  in 
excitation  experiments  could  get  no  evidence  of  a  Broca  centre 
in  the  anthropoid  apes. 

One  broad  resemblance  between  the  movements  elicited 
from  the  motor  cortex  and  spinal  reflexes  is  striking,  and  yet 
is,  I  think,  not  insisted  on  by  writers.  We  have  seen  that  the 
movements  elicitable  in  the  various  regions  as  local  reflexes 
by  stimulation  of  the  afferent  paths  of  those  regions  present 
regular  and  characteristic  quality.  Thus  stimuli  to  a  fore  limb 
induce  lifting  of  that  limb  with  flexion  at  elbow  and  retraction- 
flexion  at  shoulder;  stimuli  to  a  hind  limb  induce  drawing  up 
of  that  limb  with  flexion  at  knee,  hip,  and  dorsi-flexion  at 
ankle ;  stimuli  to  the  mouth  induce  opening  of  the  jaws,  and 
so  on.  While  these  movements  are  emphatically  evidenced  as 
local  spinal  reactions  the  overwhelming  predominance  of  their 


VIII]       RELATIONS  BETWEEN   MOVEMENTS  291 

occurrence  equally  emphasizes  the  scarcity  of  occurrence  of 
certain  other  movements  as  local  spinal  reactions.  Extension 
of  the  hind  limb  can,  it  is  true,  be  evoked  from  that  limb 
as  a  spinal  reflex  by  a  certain  special  form  of  stimulus,  but 
that  stimulus  has  to  be  applied  to  a  special  part  of  the  foot 
and  is  successful  only  after  "  spinal  shock  "  has  passed  off; 
while  the  flexion-reflex  can  be  evoked  by  various  forms  of 
stimuli  applied  practically  to  any  point  of  the  limb  surface,  and 
is  elicitable  almost  from  the  very  hour  of  spinal  transection 
onward.  So  also  I  have  occasionally  succeeded  in  evoking 
closure  instead  of  opening  of  the  jaw  by  stimulation  of  a  certain 
part  of  the  Hp  in  the  decerebrate  animal — but  even  then  the 
reflex  is  not  regularly  elicitable.  On  the  other  hand  reflex 
opening  of  the  jaws  is  easily  and  regularly  elicitable  from 
various  points  of  the  oral  surface.  Similarly  extension  of  the 
elbow  as  a  local  reflex  elicitable  by  stimuli  applied  to  the  fore 
limb  itself  is  a  reflex  practically  unknown  to  me.* 

Now  those  movements  that  are  practically  wanting  as  local 
bulbo-spinal  reflexes  and  strike  the  observer  of  the  spinal  or 
decerebrate  animal  by  their  default,  are  likewise  practically 
absent,  comparatively  infrequent,  or  only  limitedly  and  irregu- 
larly elicitable  from  the  motor  cortex  itself.^^  On  the  other 
hand,  the  movements  regularly  and  widely  elicitable  as  local 
reflexes  are  liberally  represented  in  the  motor  cortex.^^ 

In  the  light  of  the  observations  mentioned  above,  which  show 
that  reciprocal  innervation  is  a  mode  of  co-ordination  widely 
exhibited  in  the  reactions  elicitable  through  the  motor  cortex, 
this  sparse  occurrence  of  certain  movements,  e,  g.  extension  of 
the  knee  or  closure  of  the  jaw,  does  not  mean  that  the  ex- 
tensor muscles  of  the  knee  or  the  muscles  which  close  the 
jaws  are  unrepresented  in  the  cortex.  It  does  not  mean  that 
this  cortex  is  in  touch  with  the  flexors  alone  and  not  with  the 
extensors.  It  means  that  the  usual  effect  of  the  cortex  on  these 
latter  is  inhibitio7t.     It  means  not  that  the  extensors  and  the 

*  Extension  of  elbow  and  of  knee  are  of  course  easily  obtainable  as  crossed 
reflexes  and  as  parts  of  reflexes  evoked  from  distant  points.  Such  reflexes  I  do  not 
include  as  local  reflex  reactions. 


292     REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect. 

jaw-closers  are  unrepresented  cortically,  but  that  their  normal 
representation  in  the  cortex  under  the  ordinary  conditions  of 
experiment  has  the  form  of  inhibition,  not  excitation,  and  thus 
unless  specially  sought  escapes  observation. 

Since,  as  above  shown,  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  trans- 
form certain  inhibitions  into  excitations,  we  have  a  means  of 
further  testing  this  point.  It  is  in  my  experience  quite  ex- 
ceptional to  obtain  primary  extension  of  the  opposite  knee 
as  a  motor  reaction  from  the  cerebral  cortex  of  the  cat  —  or 
even,  indeed,  as  a  secondary  movement.  In  exploring  the 
cortex  with  unipolar  faradization  I  have  often  failed  to  elicit 
the  movement  at  all  throughout  a  series  of  observations. 
Flexion,  on  the  other  hand,  is  regularly  obtainable.  After 
exhibition  of  strychnine  the  extension  of  knee  can  be  regu- 
larly excited  from  the  cortex,  and  from  the  very  points  of  it 
that  yielded  flexion  previously.  This  conversion  is  not  so  facile 
as  the  conversion  of  the  spinal  reflex.  The  dose  of  strych- 
nine has  to  be  larger,  or  to  operate  longer.  With  doses  addi- 
tively  given,  there  seems,  early  in  the  experiment,  a  period  when 
reflex  spinal  inhibition  of  the  extensors  has  been  converted  into 
excitation,  but  the  cortex  of  the  brain  still  yields  knee-flexion, 
not  knee-extension.  The  cortical  reversal  has  required  in  my 
hands  doses  that  evoke  convulsive  seizures  from  time  to  time. 
I  have  seen,  immediately  after  a  severe  convulsion,  the  cortex 
either  unable  to  evoke  any  movement  of  the  knee  or  produce 
knee-flexion,  though  a  short  while  before  it  gave  knee-extension. 

Tetanus  toxin  likewise  converts  the  cortical  flexion  into 
extension.  The  efl*ect  is  in  its  case  the  more  marked,  because, 
if  the  cortical  examination  be  at  an  early  stage  of  the  progressive 
malady  ensuing  on  inoculation  by  a  moderate  dose,  or  where 
the  dose  has  been  quite  small,  the  tetanus  is  "  local ".  and  con- 
fined to  the  inoculated  limb,  and  then,  if  the  tetanus  be  "  local" 
in  one  hind  limb,  e.  g.  the  left,  the  appropriate  area  of  the  right 
hemisphere  yields  knee-extension,  whereas  the  corresponding  of 
the  left  hemisphere  yields  knee-flexion. 

But  these  effects  are  better  studied  in  the  monkey.  There, 
in  my  experience,  to  obtain  primary  extension  of  the  crossed 


■ 


II]         REPRESENTATION  OF  MOVEMENTS  293 


knee  from  the  cortex  is,  as  in  the  cat,  extremely  unusual.  A 
number  of  experiments  can  be  made  without  obtaining  it  at  all. 
Even  as  a  secondary  movement  it  is  extremely  poorly  repre- 
sented in  the  cortex.  For  twenty  instances  of  flexion  at  knee 
it  is,  in  my  experience,  often  difficult  to  find  one  of  extension  at 
that  joint.  But  after  tetanus  toxin  or  strychnine  the  whole  "  leg- 
area  "  of  the  cortex,  from  all  points  of  its  surface,  may  yield 
nothing  but  leg-extension,  in  which  extension  of  knee  is  prominent 
as  an  evident  part  of  a  primary  combined  movement.  This  is  espe- 
cially striking  when  the  tetanus  is  still  merely  **  local,"  and  con- 
fined to  one  hind  limb,  e.  g.  left.  The  **  leg-area  "  of  the  right 
cortex  then  yields  knee-extension  everywhere;  the  "  leg-area " 
of  the  left  cortex  yields  the  normal  flexion  results.  The  "  leg- 
area  "  of  the  right  cortex  provokes  moreover  from  many  of  its 
points  extension  of  right  knee  and  ankle,  as  well  as  of  left,  though 
less  strongly.  The  "  leg-area  "  of  left  hemisphere  does  this 
little,  if  at  all.  Under  moderate  faradization  the  "  leg-area  "  in 
the  monkey,  in  my  experience,  moves  the  homonymous  hind 
limb,  in  addition  to  the  crossed,  very  slightly  and  rarely,  much 
less  easily  than  in  the  cat,  though  in  both  the  movement  is  the 
same,  namely,  "  extension."  So  localized  may  be  the  toxic 
influence  in  its  early  stage  that  reversal  of  the  usual  cortical 
eflect  at  knee  may  obtain  while  in  the  same  hemisphere  that  on 
hip  and  ankle  still  remain  flexion  as  usual. 

Similarly  with  the  "  arm-area."  In  the  cat,  it  is  in  my  expe- 
rience quite  infrequent  to  obtain  primary  extension  of  the  crossed 
elbow  from  the  cortex.  Flexion  is  readily  and  regularly  obtained. 
Strychnine  changes  this :  the  very  surface  that  yielded  flexion 
then  provokes  extension,  and  strongly.  But  the  dose  of  strych- 
nine seems  to  be  larger  than  for  conversion  of  the  spinal  reflex, 
and  the  conversion  shows  the  phases  before  mentioned  in  regard 
to  the  knee-inhibition,  and  its  conversion  in  the  case  of  the  ham- 
string nerve.  In  the  monkey,  in  my  experience,  the  effect  of 
strychnine  and  of  tetanus  toxin  when  pushed  to  the  general 
convulsive  stage  is  often  contrary  to  the  effect  in  that  stage  in 
so  many  other  animals.  I  have  seen  them,  though  producing 
extension   at   elbow  at  first,  later   produce    flexion   at  elbow. 


294      REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX    [Lect. 

In  one  case,  in  a  monkey,  in  which  the  tetanus  had  become 
general  in  the  sense  that  only  one  limb  was  unaffected,  thJ|, 
affected  arm  was  strongly  extended  and  rigid  at  elbow  with 
some  retraction  at  shoulder.  But  in  all  my  instances,  where  by 
introduction  of  the  toxin  into  the  trunk  of  the  median  or  ulnar  a 
"  local  "  tetanus  of  the  arm  has  been  produced,  the  limb  has  been 
extended  rigidly  at  elbow  and  retracted  at  shoulder.  In  these 
cases  faradic  examination  of  the  cortex  showed  that  the  small 
field  of  the  "  arm-area"  to  which  extension  at  elbow  is  restricted, 
was  enlarged  so  as  to  include  the  whole  "  arm-area."  Under 
the  toxin  the  cortex  that  normally  in  the  cat  yields  flexion 
of  the  crossed  fore  limb  and  extension  of  the  uncrossed,  will 
yield  extension  of  both  when  there  is  local  tetanus  in  the 
crossed  limb.  Extension  at  elbow  sometimes  alone,  more 
often  with  retraction  at  shoulder,  or  with  extension  at  wrist 
or  fingers,  sometimes  as  a  leading  movement,  sometimes 
rapidly  ensuent  on  retraction  at  shoulder  or  extension  in  the 
hand,  according  as  higher  or  lower  points  in  the  area  were 
stimulated,  was  prominently  exhibited  at  all  points  of  the  entire 
surface  of  the  "  arm-area."  That  area,  with  this  as  its  salient 
reaction,  seemed  particularly  in  evidence,  for  its  extreme  limits 
appeared  traceable  further  than  usual,  and  to  encroach  on  or 
overlap  more  than  is  usual  under  the  feeble  or  moderate  stimu- 
lation employed,  the  "  leg-area  "  above  and  the  **  face-area  " 
below,  and  to  run  exceptionally  far  forward  above  the  pre- 
central  sulcus,  though  remaining  undemonstrable  in  the  free 
surface  of  the  ascending  parietal  convolution.  From  no  point 
in  all  this  extensive  "  arm-area  "  was,  despite  repeated  trials, 
any  flexion  at  elbow  or  shoulder  or  hand  obtained  (Fig.  75). 
Various  intensities  of  faradization  were  employed,  and  points 
known  normally  to  yield  it  most  regularly  were  tried :  but  ex- 
tension, not  flexion,  always  resulted. 

This  condition  of  the  "  arm-area  "  can  in  tetanus  exist  in  one 
hemisphere  or  even  in  both  hemispheres  and  the  "leg-area"  of 
each  hemisphere  yet  yield  flexions  at  knee  and  hip  and  ankle, 
and  its  other  normal  forms  of  reaction.  Tetanus  produced 
by  introduction  of  the  toxin  into  the  arm  (js.  g.  median  or 


1 


I]  STRYCHNINE  AND  THE   CORTEX  295 

ulnar  trunk)  affects  subsequently  to  the  inoculated  limb,  the 
fellow  fore  limb  first,  and  the  jaw  before  the  hind  limbs,  although 
the  knee-jerk  on  the  homonymous  side  to  the  inoculation  may 
be  brisk. 

Under  decerebrate  rigidity,  e,  g.  in  the  cat,  the  closing 
muscles  of  the  jaw  are  kept  in  tonic  action,  holding  the  mouth 
somewhat  shut.^^  By  stimulation  of  any  point  of  a  large  **  skin- 
area  "  appropriate  for  the  reflex,  reflex  opening  of  the  mouth, 
including  depression  of  the  lower  jaw,  is  easily  and  regularly 
elicited,  or  by  faradization  of  an  afferent  twig  of  the  trigeminus; 
or  as  was  shown  by  Woodworth  and  myself,^"^  even  by  stimula- 
tion of  distant  afferent  nerves,  e,  g.  plantar  or  saphenous.  Here 
the  action  of  the  powerful  closing  muscles  is  reflexly  inhibited 
while  the  weaker  opening  muscles  are  reflexly  excited  —  it 
seems,  in  fact,  a  case  of  Astacus  claw,  except  that  the  inhibi- 
tion is  central,  not  peripheral.  This  reflex  **  opening "  is  in 
the  decerebrate  animal  converted  into  reflex  closure  by  tetanus 
toxin  and  by  strychnine,  the  inhibition  of  the  predominantly 
powerful  closing  muscles  being  converted  into  excitation  of 
them. 

Similarly,  when  the  "  face-area "  of  the  monkey's  cortex  is 
tested  by  faradization  after  exhibition  of  strychnine  the  points 
of  surface  that  previously  yielded  regularly  the  free  opening  of 
the  jaw,  yield  strong  closure  of  the  jaw  instead.  Now  closure  of 
the  jaw  is  a  movement  of  very  limited  representation  in  the  cor- 
tex of  the  monkey,  even  of  the  anthropoid.  On  the  other  hand, 
opening  of  the  jaw  is  always  readily  and  regularly  elicitable  from 
a  large  field  of  the  "  face-area."  And  adjoining  and  overlapping 
this  large  area  whence  steady  opening  of  the  jaw  is  obtained,  is 
found  an  area  whence,  as  Ferrier '^  first  pointed  out,  "  rhythmic 
alternating  opening  and  closing  of  the  jaws,"  as  in  feeding,  can 
be  evoked.  Under  tetanus  toxin  (Fig.  75)  and  strychnine  the 
whole  of  this  combined  area  not  only  ceases  to  yield  opening  of 
the  jaws,  either  maintained  or  rhythmic,  but  yields  closing  of 
them  instead  —  often  with  visible  retraction  of  the  tongue.  For 
this  conversion  larger  doses  of  strychnine  have,  in  my  hands, 
been  required  than  for  conversion  of  knee-flexion  into  extension. 


296     REACTIONS   OF   THE   MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect.     j 

With  tetanus  toxin  the  conversion  appears  the  more  striking 
when  examined  early  in  the  progress  of  the  intoxication,  because 
it  may  be  found  at  a  stage  preceding  altogether  the  occurrence  ■ 
of  any  general  convulsions,  and  also  because  it  can  then  some- 
times be  found  to  be  unilateral,  that  is,  to  be  present  in  the 
"  face-area  "  of  one  hemisphere  *  without  or  almost  without  any 
affection  of  the  "  face-area "  of  the  other  hemisphere.  The 
reactions  of  the  normal  field  thus  remain  for  comparison  in  the 
same  individual  with  the  reactions  of  the  abnormal  field. 

Tetanus  toxin   shows  marked  predilection    for  the  closure    l 
mechanism  of  the  jaw.     After  inoculation  in  the  hind  leg,  even 
before  the  "  local "  tetanus  has  obviously  invaded  the  fellow 
limb  of  the  opposite   side,  a   slight  tightness  of  jaw  and  an    ] 
immobile  pursing  of  the  lips  has  several  times  given  warning     i 
that  general   tetanus  had   really   set  in,    before   any   trace   of 
general  convulsive  seizures  or   any  involvement  of  the  arms     i 
was  detected.     Tetanus  toxin  has  also  certainly  intensified  the     | 
reactions  of  the  cortical  areas  that  give  retraction  of  the  neck     ; 
and  retraction  of  the  abdominal  wall  (Fig.  75). 

The  progress  of  the  change  wrought  by  these  agents  in  con- 
verting these  reactions  of  the  cortex  from  their  usual  form  to     j 
the  diametrically  opposed  seems  to  involve  the   same  kind  of    ' 
steps  as  that  noted  above  in  their  conversion  of  the  inhibitory     ; 
hamstring    nerve  effect  on  the  knee-extensor.     Stages  can  be 
found  in  which  the  inhibitory  effect  is  less  than  normal,  yet  is 
not  replaced  by  excitatory.     With  the  cortical  opening  of  jaw, 
in  early  tetanus  a  grade  is  discoverable  when  faradization  of  the 
cortex  produces  a  slight  opening  of  the  jaw  —  a  mere  *'  loosen- 
ing" of  the  jaws,  so  to  say  —  distinctly  less  than  normal,  and     j 
hardly  effectively  opening  the  mouth.    Also  with  the  "  leg-area  " 
of  the  cortex,  at  an  early  stage  of  the  tetanus  it  would  seem  that    ';j 
an  undue  but  far  from  exclusive  preponderance  of  plantar  exten- 
sion at  ankle  over  dorsal  flexion  at  that  joint  exists,  while  the     ! 
symptomatic  knee-extension  is  as  yet  not  excitable  though  knee- 

*  The  hemisphere,  the  "  face-area  "  of  which  is  earlier  affected,  is,  in  the  case  of 
inoculation  in  a  limb,  the  hemisphere  contralateral  to  the  limb  inoculated!  .| 


Ill]    CORTICAL   ANTAGONISTIC    MOVEMENTS    297 


BODV 

_,   tir»tlKefoni< 


I.EO - 


ARM    c 


FACE 


LEQ 


ARM    - 


BODY 


NECK 


FACE 


Pig.  75 .  —  Outline  to  illustrate  the  changes  produced  by  tetanus-toxin  in  the  functional  topog- 
raphy of  the  motor-cortex  of  the  monkey,  Cercopithecus  callithrix.  CF  =  the  central 
fissure.  F  indicates  hip-knee  flexion,  E  indicates  hip-knee  extension  ;  /  indicates  elbow 
flexion,  e  indicates  elbow  extension ;  <  indicates  jaw  opening,  =  indicates  jaw  closing ; 
prosthotonic  indicates  in  regard  to  "body  "  ventral  bending,  opisthotonic  indicates  dorsal 
bending.  The  distribution  of  these  symbols  in  the  drawing  indicates  broadly  the  field 
whence  could  be  elicited  the  movements  that  the  symbols  respectively  stand  for,  in  A 
before  and  in  B  after  development  of  lockjaw.  In  B  at  the  lowest  part  of  the  face-area  a 
place  still  yielded  opening  of  the  mouth.  In  the  experiment  which  furnished  the  specimen 
figured  the  site  of  inoculation  had  been  the  leg,  hence  the  toxic  action  reached  the  jaw 
comparatively  late.  Had  the  exploration  of  the  cortex  been  deferred  even  longer  the  open 
ing  of  the  jaw  might  perhaps  have  been  transformed  to  closure  throughout  the  cortex. 


298      REACTIONS  OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX     [Lect. 

flexion  is  almost  in  abeyance.  Neither  under  tetanus  toxin  or 
strychnine  have  I  at  present  observed  conversion  of  the  abducens 
inhibition  into  excitation. 

The  foregoing  observations  appear  to  give  an  insight  into  at 
least  a  part  of  the  essential  nature  of  the  condition  brought 
about  by  tetanus  and  by  strychnine  poisoning.  These  disorders 
work  havoc  with  the  co-ordinating  mechanisms  of  the  central 
nervous  system  because  in  regard  to  certain  great  groups  of 
musculature  they  change  the  reciprocal  inhibitions,  normally  as- 
sured by  the  central  nervous  mechanisms,  into  excitations.  The 
sufferer  is  subjected  to  a  disorder  of  co-ordination  which,  though 
not  necessarily  of  itself  accompanied  by  physical  pain,  inflicts 
on  the  mind,  which  still  remains  clear,  a  disability  inexpressibly 
distressing.  Each  attempt  to  execute  certain  muscular  acts  of 
vital  importance,  such  as  the  taking  of  food,  is  defeated  because 
from  the  attempt  results  an  act  exactly  the  opposite  to  that 
intended.  The  endeavour  to  open  the  jaw  to  take  food  or  drink 
induces  closure  of  the  jaw,  because  the  normal  inhibition  of  the 
stronger  set  of  muscles  —  the  closing  muscles  —  is  by  the  agent 
converted  into  excitation  of  them.  Moreover,  the  various  reflex- 
arcs  that  cause  inhibition  of  these  muscles  not  only  cause  exci- 
tation of  them  instead,  but  are,  periodically  or  more  or  less 
constantly,  in  a  state  of  super-excitement,  and  yet  attempt  on 
the  part  of  the  sufferer  to  restrain,  to  inhibit,  their  reflex  reaction, 
instead  of  relaxing  them,  only  heightens  their  excitation  further, 
and  thus  exacerbates  a  rigidity  or  a  convulsion  already  in 
progress. 

It  seems  to  me  not  improbable  that  the  virus  of  rabies  may 
similarly  upset  reciprocal  innervation,  though  its  field  of  opera- 
tion, at  least  in  man,  lies  not  in  the  same  group  of  mechanisms 
as  are  affected  by  strychnine  and  tetanus  toxin  but  in  an 
allied  one,  namely,  that  inter-regulating  (by  co-ordinations  in- 
volving inhibition,  as  Meltzer  and  Kronecker  showed)  the  acts 
of  deglutition  and  respiration. 

Little  has  met  me  in  the  course  of  observations  on  the  re- 
actions of  the  cortex  under  strychnine  or  tetanus  toxin  to  indi- 
cate that  the  transformation  of  the  motor  effects  of  the  reactions 


VIII]  LOCKJAW  299 

is  due  to  action  of  these  agents  on  the  cortex  itself.  The  change 
of  result  seems  quite  explicable  by  alteration  produced  in  lower 
centres,  e.  g.  spinal  and  bulbar,  on  which  the  cortex  acts.  This 
seems  especially  shown  by  the  toxin  when  injected  into  the  right 
arm  and  producing  extensor  rigidity  at  that  elbow  and  rigid 
torticollis  to  the  right,  converting  the  flexion  of  arm-area  of  the 
left  hemisphere  into  extension  under  arm-area  excitation,  and 
in  the  right  hemisphere  torticollis  movement  to  the  right. 

The  vast  role  of  inhibition  in  cerebral  processes  as  evidenced 
by  mental  reactions,  and  the  slightness  of  mental  disorder  in 
strychnine  poisoning  or  tetanus  indicates  a  difference  between 
inhibition  as  it  occurs  in  the  bulbo-spinal  arcs  and  in  the  arcs 
of  purely  sensual  and  perceptual  level,  a  difference  presumably 
of  physicochemical  nature. 

We  find,  therefore,  these  reactions  changed  in  a  like  manner 
by  strychnine  and  tetanus  whether  we  excite  them  from  the 
cortex  or  from  reflex  spinal  arcs.  And  a  further  similarity  be- 
tween the  representation  of  movement  in  the  motor  cortex  and 
in  the  bulbo-spinal  axis  as  a  mechanism  for  local  reflexes  is  the 
following.  When  the  induced  movement  embraces  both  hind 
limbs  or  both  fore  limbs  it  is  in  an  opposite  sense  in  the  two 
limbs.  Thus  the  crossed  accompaniment  to  the  flexion-reflex 
of  the  limb  is  extension :  and  so  also  when  cortical  stimulation 
evokes  [e.g.  in  cat)  flexion  e.g.  of  the  right  fore  limb,  not  rarely 
it  evokes  movement,  weaker  it  is  true,  in  the  left,  and  that  move- 
ment, as  Exner^  noted  in  the  rabbit,  is  extension. 

The  local  reflex  movements  obtainable  from  the  bulbo-spinal 
animal  and  the  reactions  elicitable  from  the  motor  cortex  of  the 
narcotized  animal  fall  into  line  as  similar  series.  Both  consist 
of  the  same  group.  But  in  striking  contrast  to  this  group  stands 
the  motor  innervation  active  in  "  decerebrate  rigidity." 

Decerebrate  rigidity  ^^^  is  a  condition  which  ensues  on  remo- 
val of  the  fore-brain  by  transection  at  any  of  the  various  levels 
in  the  mesencephalon  or  the  thalamencephalon  in  its  hinder  part. 

If  in  a  monkey  or  cat  transection  below  or  in  the  lower  half 
of  the  bulb  has  been  performed,  the  animal  when  suspended, 


300      REACTIONS   OF   THE   MOTOR   CORTEX     [Lect. 

artificial  respiration  if  necessary  being  kept  up,  hangs  from  the 
suspension  points  with  deeply  drooped  neck,  deeply  drooped 
tail,  and  its  pendent  limbs  flaccid  and  slightly  flexed.  The  fore 
limb  is  slightly  flexed  at  shoulder,  at  elbow,  and  very  slightly 
at  wrist.  The  hind  limb  is  slightly  flexed  at  hip,  at  knee,  and 
at  ankle.  On  giving  the  hand  or  foot  a  push  forward  and  then 
releasing  it,  the  limb  swings  back  into  and  somewhat  beyond  the 
position  of  its  equilibrium  under  gravity;  and  it  oscillates  a  few 
times  backward  and  forward  before  finally  settling  down  to  its 
original  position. 

To  this  condition  of  flaccid  paralysis  supervening  upon  tran- 
section in  the  lower  half  of  the  bulb  the  condition  ensuing  on 
removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  offers  a  great  contrast.  In 
the  latter  case  the  animal,  on  being  suspended  just  as  after  the 
former  operation,  hangs  with  its  fore  limbs  thrust  backward,  with 
retraction  at  shoulder  joint,  straightened  elbow,  and  some  flexion 
at  wrist.  The  hand  of  the  monkey  is  turned  with  its  palmar 
face  somewhat  inward.  The  hind  limbs  are  similarly  straight- 
ened and  thrust  backward ;  the  hip  is  extended,  the  knee  very 
stiffly  extended,  and  the  ankle  somewhat  extended.  The  tail 
in  spite  of  its  own  weight,  and  it  is  quite  heavy  in  some  species 
of  monkey,  is  kept  either  straight  and  horizontal  or  often  stiffly 
curved  upward.  There  is  a  little  opisthotonus  of  the  lumbo- 
sacral vertebral  region.  The  head  is  kept  lifted  against  gravity 
and  the  chin  is  tilted  upward  under  the  retraction  and  backward 
rotation  of  the  skull  on  the  neck.  The  mouth  is  kept  closed 
and  there  is  some  stiffness  in  the  elevators  of  the  jaw.  When 
the  limbs  or  tail  or  head  or  jaw  are  pushed  from  the  pose  they 
have  assumed  considerable  resistance  to  the  movement  is  felt, 
and  unHke  the  condition  after  bulbar  section,  on  being  released 
they  spring  back  at  once  to  their  former  position  and  remain 
there  for  a  time  more  rigid  than  before. 

The  rigidity  is  immediately  due  to  prolonged  spasm  of  cer- 
tain groups  of  voluntary  muscles.  The  chief  of  these  are  the 
retractor  muscles  of  the  head  and  neck,  the  elevators  of  the  jaw 
and  tail,  and  the  extensor  muscles  of  the  elbow  and  knee,  and 
shoulder  {i,e,  deltoids)  and  hip.     In  the  dog  and  cat,  just  as 


VIII]  DECEREBRATE   RIGIDITY  301 

spinal  shock  is  more  severe  in  the  fore  limbs  than  in  the  hind,  so 
decerebrate  rigidity  is  more  marked  in  the  fore  than  in  the 
hind  limb.  This  prolonged  spasm  may  be  maintained,  with 
intermissions,  for  a  period  of  four  days.  It  is  increased,  and 
even  when  absent  or  very  slight,  may  be  soon  developed  by 
passive  movements  of  the  part.  There  is  no  obvious  tremor 
in  the  spasm  in  the  earlier  hours  of  its  continuance;  later  it 
does  sometimes  become  tremulant. 

Administration  of  chloroform  and  ether,  if  carried  far,  quite 
abolishes  the  rigidity.  On  interrupting  the  administration  the 
rigidity  again  rapidly  returns. 

Section  of  the  dorsal  columns  of  the  spinal  cord  does  not\ 
abolish  the  rigidity.  Section  of  one  lateral  column  of  the  cord 
in  the  upper  lumbar  region  abolishes  the  rigidity  in  the  hind 
limb  of  the  same  side  as  the  section.  Section  of  one  ventro- 
lateral column  of  the  cord  in  the  cervical  region  destroys  the 
rigidity  in  the  fore  and  hind  limbs  of  the  same  side. 

The  rigidity  develops  either  very  imperfectly  or  not  at  all 
in  a  limb  the  afferent  roots  of  which  have  been  severed  some 
days  prior  to  carrying  out  the  operation  which  produces  the 
rigidity. 

If  after  ablation  of  both  cerebral  hemispheres,  even  when 
the  rigidity  is  being  maintained  at  its  extreme  height,  the  affer- 
ent roots  previously  laid  bare  and  prepared,  are  carefully  severed, 
the  limb  at  once  falls  flaccid.  The  result  is  quite  local,  that  is, 
confined  to  the  one  limb  the  afferent  roots  of  which  are  severed. 

Decerebrate  rigidity  exhibits  reflex  excitation  in  those  very 
groups  of  muscles  which  the  local  reflexes  and  the  motor  cortex 
when  stimulated  excite  but  little.  Not  that  the  muscles  exhibit- 
ing the  rigidity  are  absolutely  unamenable  to  transient  spinal 
reflexes.  The  extensor- thrust  and  certain  crossed  reflexes  are 
witness  to  the  contrary.  And  they  are  not  absolutely  unamen- 
able to  cortical  excitation.  The  extension  of  the  elbow  obtain- 
able from  the  cortex  refutes  that.  But  these  instances  do  not 
efface  the  broad  fact  that  a  wide  system  of  musculature,  includ- 
ing the  extensors  of  the  hip,  knee,  shoulder,  and  elbow,  and 
the  elevators  of  the   tail,  neck,  and  jaw,  is  inhibited  by  the 


302     REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR   CORTEX    [Lect. 

overwhelming  majority  of  local  spinal  reflexes  and  of  reactions 
from  the  motor  cortex,  but  on  the  contrary  is  excited  in  a  set 
of  reflex  reactions  which  employ  the  local  deep  afferents  (pro- 
prioceptive) and  some  cranial  mechanism  seated  between  cere- 
brum and  bulb.     The  cerebellum  at  once  rises  to  mind.     But  ll 
found  ablation  of  the  cerebellum  did  not  abolish  the  rigidity. 
It  is  significant  that  a  vertical  posture  favors  the  appearance 
and  development  of  the  rigidity.     The  muscles  it  predominantly 
affects    are   those   which   in   that   attitude    antagonize    gravity. 
In  standing,  walking,  running,  the  limbs  would  sink  under  the 
body's  weight  but  for  contraction  of  the  extensors  of  hip,  knee, 
ankle,  shoulder,  elbow;  the  head  would  hang  but  for  the  re- 
tractors of  the  neck ;  the  tail  and  jaw  would  drop  but  for  their 
elevator  muscles.     These  muscles  counteract  a  force  (gravity) 
that   continually  threatens  to  upset  the  natural  posture.     The 
force  acts  continuously  and  the  muscles  exhibit  continued  action,  ^ 
tonus.     We  seem  to  have  here  a  field  of  muscles  combined  as  ; 
a  physiological  entity.      A   characteristic  reaction  yielded  by  ; 
muscles  of  this  field  is  the  **  jerk,"  the  "  tendon  phenomenon,"  | 
itself  a  sign  of  highly  maintained  reflex  tonus.  J 

Two  separable  systems  of  motor  innervation  appear  thus  I 
controlling  two  sets  of  musculature:  one  system  exhibits 
those  transient  phases  of  heightened  reaction  which  constitute 
reflex  movements;  the  other  maintains  that  steady  tonic  re-  ; 
sponse  which  supplies  the  muscular  tension  necessary  to  j 
attitude.  Starting  from  the  tonic  innervation  as  initial  state  \ 
the  first  step  in  movement  tends  to  be  flexion  and  involves  j 
under  **  reciprocal  innervation "  an  inhibition  of  the  extensor  ] 
excitation  then  in  process.  This  will  be  involved  whether  the  \ 
excitation  be  via  local  reflex  or  via  the  motor  cortex.  Hence  : 
the  very  muscles  that  to  the  observer  are  most  obviously  under  \ 
excitation  by  the  tonic  system  are  those  most  obviously  in-  ; 
hibited  by  the  phasic  reflex  system.  And  the  tonic  system  '■■ 
will,  on  inhibition  of  it  passing  off",  contribute  toward  a  return  ^ 
movement  to  the  pre-existing  pose,  thus  having  its  share  in  , 
alternating  movements  and  in  compensatory  reflexes.  These  \ 
two  systems,  the  tonic  and  the  phasic  reflex  systems,  co-operate   \ 

I 


VIII]  DECEREBRATE   RIGIDITY  303 

exerting  influences  complemental  to  each  other  upon  various 
units  of  the  musculature.  Drugs  and  other  agents  that  act 
in  a  selective  way  upon  nervous  processes  might  be  expected 
in  some  cases  to  throw  into  relief  the  operation  of  one  or 
other  member  of  this  paired  system.  Strychnine  and  tetanus 
toxin  administered  to  an  animal  in  decerebrate  rigidity  increase 
that  rigidity.  The  posture  assumed  by  the  limbs,  neck,  tail, 
head,  etc.,  in  strychnine  poisoning  and  in  tetanus  resembles 
closely  in  many  respects  the  attitude  of  decerebrate  rigidity. 
There  are  differences,  —  for  instance,  the  ankle  is  often  rigidly 
extended  in  tetanus  whereas  it  is  little  affected  in  decerebrate 
rigidity ;  nevertheless  there  is  much  general  resemblance. 

And  just  as  certain  agents  display  their  action  more  obvi- 
ously in  one  member  of  these  paired  systems  than  in  the  other 
so  processes  of  disease  may  be  expected  to  deal  with  the  two 
systems  ^^equally  and  to  reveal  more  obviously  and  affect 
more  deeply  one  of  them  than  the  other.  Hughlings  Jack- 
son'^'®»^^  with  characteristic  penetration  of  thought  argued 
nearly  thirty  years  ago  that  rigidity  ensuing  in  hemiplegia 
(hemiplegic  contracture)  is  not  owing  to  the  cerebral  lesion 
nor  to  the  lateral  sclerosis.  He  said :  "  Whilst  the  primary  cere- 
bral lesion  can  account  for  the  paralytic  element  it  cannot  (nor 
can  the  sclerosis  of  the  lateral  column)  account  for  the  tonic 
condition  of  the  muscles.  My  speculation  is  that  the  rigidity  is 
owing  to  unantagonized  influence  of  the  cerebellum.  Whilst 
the  cerebrum  innervates  the  muscles  in  the  order  of  their  action 
from  the  most  voluntary  movements  (limbs)  to  the  most  auto- 
matic (trunk),  the  cerebellum  innervates  them  in  the  opposite 
order.  This  is  equivalent  to  saying  that  the  cerebellum  is  the 
centre  for  continuous  movements  and  the  cerebrum  for  chang- 
ing movements.  Thus  in  *  walking '  the  cerebellum  tends  to 
stiffen  all  the  muscles ;  the  changing  movements  of  walking  are 
the  result  of  ceiebral  discharges  overcoming  in  a  particular  and 
orderly  way  the  otherwise  continuous  cerebellar  influence. 
When  the  influence  of  the  cerebrum  is  permanently  taken  off 
by  disease  of  the  cerebrum,  as  in  hemiplegia,  from  the  parts 
which  it  most  specially  governs  (arm  and  leg)  the  cerebellar 


304    REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX     [Lect.  ^ 

influence  is  no  longer  antagonized;  there  is  unimpeded  cere-  I 
bellar  influx  and  hence  rigidity  of  the  muscles  which  in  health  i 
the  cerebrum  chiefly  innervates.  The  spinal  muscles  are  those 
which  the  cerebrum  influences  least  and  the  cerebellum  most.  'I 
In  health  the  whole  of  the  muscles  of  the  body  are  doubly  I 
innervated — innervated  both  by  the  cerebrum  and  cerebel-  I 
lum :  there  being  a  co-operation  of  antagonism  between  the  j 
two  great  centres."  i 

This  view  of  Hughlings  Jackson  seems  supported  and  i 
amplified  by  Luciani  and  Stefani's  work  on  the  cerebellum.  ^ 
Wernicke,^^^^  Mann,^^*  and  Lewandowski  ^^^  also  point  out  that  j 
cerebral  paresis  selects  one  group  of  antagonistic  groups  of  i 
muscles  in  the  limbs.  We  may  very  likely  have  to  seek  in  the  i 
afferent  nerves  of  muscles — especially  of  those  antagonizing  i 
gravity — and  in  the  nerve  of  the  otic  labyrinth  —  the  "  tonus- ^ 
labyrinth"  of  Ewald  —  the  sources  of  the  influence  to  which* 
HughHngs  Jackson  refers  as  "cerebellar,"  but  that  does  not  i 
radically  aflect  in  its  main  feature  the  scheme  he  draws  of  a" 
changeful  **  clonic  "  (I  would  prefer  to  say  "  phasic")  innervations 
and  a  relatively  unchanging  tonic  innervation  as  two  systems  in  I 
co-operative  antagonism.  Although  we  must  also  admit  that  ^ 
the  cortical  innervation,  pre-eminently  phasic  though  it  be,  also  ^ 
is  to  some  extent  tonic ;  Lewandowski's  ^^  study  of  hemiplegic  i 
contracture  seems  to  make  this  certain. 

And  here  arises  a  question  concerning  the  neural  tonus  ] 
of  the  skeletal  musculature.  Since  Brondgeest's  experiment  t 
neural  tonus  has  been  demonstrated  to  exist  in  various  muscles  i 
and  to  be  of  reflex  origin.  It  has  however  remained  a  question^ 
whether  a//  skeletal  muscles  habitually  exhibit  a  reflex  tonus  | 
or  only  some  of  them.  Various  experiments  (Heidenhain,  i 
Wundt)  failed  to  discover  reflex  tonus  in  the  muscles  examined  i 
by  them.  If  the  reciprocal  innervation  of  antagonistic  muscles  ] 
which  obtains  in  so  many  reflexes  obtains  also  in  the  tonic  ^ 
reflexes  maintaining  neural  tonus  in  muscles,  it  is  obvious  thati 
when  one  muscle  of  an  antagonistic  pair  exhibits  reflex  tonus  I 
its  antagonist  will  not  exhibit  reflex  tonus,  but  on  the  contrary  | 
a  slight  degree  of  reflex  inhibition.     We  have  as  yet  no  clear  | 


VIII]     REFLEX  SYSTEMS  — TONIC  AND  PHASIC     305 

evidence  on  this.  The  feeble  steady  excitation  which  is  the 
sign  of  reflex  tonus  is  often  difficult  to  demonstrate.  Feeble 
steady  inhibition  would  be  even  less  easy  to  detect.  But 
the  selective  distribution  of  the  jerk-phenomena,  under  the 
ordinary  conditions  employed  for  their  elicitation,  to  single 
members  of  antagonistic  couples  e.  g.^  glutaeiis^  vasto-crureus , 
masseter,  and  their  absence,  under  those  conditions,  from  the 
opposite  members  of  the  couples,  is  suggestive  that,  under  the 
condition  taken,  reflex  tonus  may  be  confined  to  one  member 
of  each  antagonistic  pair,  namely  to  that  member  which  is  then 
in  reflex  tonic  operation,  e,  g.  counteracting  gravity  for  the 
preservation  of  an  habitual  pose  of  the  animal. 

I  have  laid  some  stress  on  the  broad  resemblance  between 
the  movements  elicitable  from  the  motor  cortex  and  those  of 
local  spinal  reflexes.     There  are  broad  differences  as  well. 

Spinal  reflex  movements  suggest  fairly  obviously  protective, 
procreative,  or  visceral  functions  on  the  one  hand,  and  on  the 
other  the  main  movements  of  the  progression  habitual  to  the 
animal.  They  seem  to  refer  to  stimulation  of  noci-ceptive  or 
sexual  skin  nerves  or  visceral  afferent  fibres,  as  though  initiated 
by  these.  They  carry  little  unequivocal  reference  to  "  touch." 
The  existence  of  spinal  reflexes  elicited  by  pure  "  touch "  — 
apart  from  that  noxious  touch  evoking  scratching-reflexes  or 
eye-blinking  —  appears  to  me  not  established  in  respect  to  the 
normal  spinal  cord.  Similarly  in  the  cat  and  dog  after  decere- 
bration  no  purely  auditory  stimulus  in  my  experience  excites 
a  reflex,*  nor  do  visual,  though  the  optic  tracts  and  their  mid- 
brain connections  have  been  spared  in  the  decerebration.  On 
the  other  hand  various  movements  elicitable  from  the  motor 
cortex  carry  the  significance  of  possible  responses  to  tactual, 
auditory,  or  visual  stimuli ;  for  instance,  the  closure  of  the  hand, 
the  pricking  of  the  ear,  the  opening  of  the  eyes,  and  turning  of 
the  head  in  the  direction  of  the  gaze. 

Combination  of  cortical  reaction  with  spinal  reflex  seems 
patent  in  certain  reactions  of  the  dog.     Thus,  the  normal  dog 

*  I  have  only  seen  it  do  so  when  the  decerebrate  animal  has  been  under  large 
doses  of  atropin. 


3o6     REACTIONS   OF  THE   MOTOR  CORTEX    [Lect. 

can  be  seen  to,  as  it  were,  release,  direct,  and  cut  short  a 
scratching  reflex  (v.  s.  p.  289).  Darwin^  draws  attention  to 
a  phase  of  canine  behavior  in  regard  to  defaecation.  **  Dogs 
after  voiding  their  excrement  often  make  with  all  four  feet  a 
few  scratches  backward,  even  on  a  bare  stone  pavement,  as  if 
for  the  purpose  of  covering  up  their  excrement  with  earth,  in 
nearly  the  same  manner  as  do  cats."  In  the  spinal  dog  defae- 
cation is  similarly  followed  by  a  number  of  vigorous  backward 
kicks  with  the  hind  limbs.  The  fore  limbs  I  have  not  been  able 
to  observe  because  the  spinal  transection  has  not  lain  far  enough 
headward  to  Hberate  those  limbs  for  free  reflex  action.  But  this 
movement  in  the  hind  limb  follows  as  a  reflex  in  the  spinal  dog 
practically  invariably  in  immediate  sequence  to  reflex  evacua- 
tion of  the  faeces.  In  the  normal  dog  it  is,  as  Darwin  remarked, 
not  invariable ;  and  it  is  often  not  in  immediate  sequence  to  the 
evacuation.  The  reflex  evidently  shows  modification  by  cerebral 
direction  and  control. 

Finally,  it  seems  to  me  that  the  number  of  reflex  actions 
which  are  "  neutral "  to  each  other,  in  the  sense  expressed  in 
Lecture  II,  is  less  with  the  cerebral  cortex  present  than  without 
it.  This  amounts  to  expressing  concretely  an  inference  that  the 
cerebral  cortex  augments  the  motor  solidarity  of  the  creature. 
Since  there  is  more  solidarity  as  well  as  more  diversity  in  those 
movements  of  an  animal  which  are  directed  to  its  outer  environ- 
ment than  to  its  inner  —  meaning  by  this  latter  the  fraction  of 
environment  embraced  within  its  own  pulmono-digestive  cavity 
—  the  representation  of  visceral  movement  in  the  cortex  will 
be  relatively  slight  and  chiefly  concern  parts  where  alimentary 
canal  opens  on  outer  surface. 

The  reactions  of  receptor-organs  which  respond  to  stimuli 
from  a  distance  tend  especially  to  have  large  cortical  representa- 
tion. These  receptors  tend  more  than  others  to  control  the 
skeletal  musculature  of  the  creature  as  a  whole.  The  contribu- 
tion made  by  the  cerebral  hemispheres  to  the  solidarity  of  the 
motor  creature  is  largely  traceable  to  their  bringing  to  bear  on 
other  reflexes  the  unifying  influence  of  the  reactions  of  the 
"  distance-receptors y     This  statement  may  in  its  baldness  appear 


VIII]  GUIDANCE  OF  SPINAL  REFLEXES  307 

doctrinaire ;  of  that  character  I  hope  to  relieve  it  somewhat  in 
the  next  following  Lecture. 

As  to  the  meaning  of  this  whole  class  of  movements  elicitable 
from  the  so-called  '*  motor "  cortex,  whether  they  represent  a 
step  toward  psychical  integration  or  on  the  other  hand  express 
the  motor  result  of  psychical  integration,  or  are  participant  in 
both,  is  a  question  of  the  highest  interest,  but  one  which  does 
not  seem  as  yet  to  admit  of  satisfactory  answer.  In  regard  to 
the  relatively  restricted  problem  in  view  in  these  lectures, 
namely,  the  simpler  elements  of  the  nervous  integration  of 
animal  reaction,  the  motor  reactions  elicitable  from  the  so- 
called  "  motor  "  cortex  furnish  evidence  confirmatory  of  points 
mentioned  before  in  regard  to  lower  reflex  action.  This  is  in- 
teresting, since  they  must  be  admitted  to  be  movements  of  higher 
order  than  any  of  those  others.  Nevertheless  they  are  to  my 
thinking  merely  fractional  movements ;  movements  which  rep- 
resent but  parts  of  the  nervous  discharge  which  emanates  from 
the  brain  under  the  normal  working  of  its  unmutilated  whole. 
The  results  before  you  must  appear  a  meagre  contribution 
toward  the  greater  problems  of  the  working  of  the  brain ;  their 
very  poverty  may  help  to  emphasize  the  necessity  for  resorting 
to  new  methods  of  experimental  inquiry  in  order  to  advance 
in  this  field.  New  methods  of  promise  seem  to  me  those  lately 
followed  by  Franz,  Thorndyke,  Yerkes,  and  others ;  for  instance, 
the  influence  of  experimental  lesions  of  the  cortex  on  skilled 
actions  recently  and  individually,  /.  e,  experientially,  acquired. 
Despite  a  protest  ably  voiced  by  v.  Uexkiill,  comparative 
psychology  seems  not  only  a  possible  experimental  science 
but  an  existent  one.  By  combining  methods  of  comparative 
psychology  {e.  g.  the  labyrinth  test)  with  the  methods  of 
experimental  physiology,  investigation  may  be  expected  ere 
long  to  furnish  new  data  of  importance  toward  the  knowledge 
of  movement  as  an  outcome  of  the  working  of  the  brain. 


3o8  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 


LECTURE    IX 

THE   PHYSIOLOGICAL  POSITION   AND   DOMINANCE 
OF  THE   BRAIN 

Argument:  The  primitive  reflex-arc.  The  diffuse  nervous  system  and 
the  gray-centred  nervous  system  ;  the  central  nervous  system  a  part 
of  the  latter.  Nervous  integration  of  the  segment.  The  three  re- 
ceptive fields.  Richness  of  the  extero-ceptive  field.  Special  refine- 
ments of  the  receptor-organs  of  the  "leading"  segments.  The 
refined  receptors  of  the  leading  segments  are  "distance-receptors." 
"  Distance-receptors ;  "  the  projicience  of  sensations.  Extensive 
internuncial  paths  belonging  to  "distance-receptors."  "Distance- 
receptors  "  initiate  precurrent  reactions.  Consummatory  reactions ; 
strong  affective  tone  of  the  sensations  adjunct  to  them.  Receptive 
range  and  locomotion.  The  "  head  "  as  physiologically  conceived. 
Proprio-ceptive  arcs  excited  secondarily  to  other  arcs.  Close  func- 
tional connection  between  the  centripetal  impulses  from  muscles  and 
from  the  labyrinth.  Tonic  reflexes  (of  posture,  etc.)  and  compen- 
satory reflexes  are  characteristic  reactions  of  this  combined  system. 
Nervous  integration  of  the  segmental  series.  Restriction  of  seg- 
mental distribution  a  factor  in  integration.  The  cerebellum  is  the 
main  ganglion  of  the  proprio-ceptive  system.  The  cerebrum  is  the 
ganglion  of  the  "distance-receptors." 

We  may  now  attempt  to  gather  from  the  various  notions,  how- 
ever fragmentary,  that  have  occupied  us,  some  general  con- 
ception of  the  neural  architecture  of  an  animal  as  a  whole; 
though  of  course  only  in  its  motor  aspect,  for  its  truly  sensorial 
aspects  we  have  hardly  had  before  us.  The  problem  is  too 
difficult  for  me  to  expect  much  success.  Yet  it  will  repay  us  if 
from  the  attempt  we  glean  something  at  least  of  one  cardinal 
feature  of  the  scheme,  namely,  the  dominance  attained  by  one 
limited  set  of  neural  segments,  the  brain,  over  all  the  rest. 

We  must  allow  ourselves  at  certain  points  some  repetition  of 
considerations  already  urged,  in  order  to  draw  from  them  now 
in  new  juxtaposition  some  further  significance. 

The  primitive  reflex-arc.  —  If  we  seek  for  a  reflex-arc  of  sim- 
plest construction  it  is  true  we  find  in  some  unicellular  organisms, 


IX]  THE   PRIMITIVE   REFLEX-ARC  309 

e.g.  Vorticella,  a  mechanism  which  resembles  a  nervous  arc  and 
is  quite  simple.  This  mechanism,  composed  from  a  single  cell, 
shows  differentiation  into  three  parts  respectively,  —  receptive^ 
conductive,  and  effective.  In  Vorticella  the  receptive  element  is 
the  ciliated  peristome ;  a  stimulus  reaching  these  cilia  at  the 
free  end  of  the  cell  excites  contraction  of  the  myoid  filament  at 
the  fixed  end  of  the  cell.  Similarly  with  the  individual  cells  of 
Poteriodendron  (Verworn).  In  multicellular  organisms  of  low 
organization  like  mechanisms  occur.  In  Actinia  there  are 
ectoderm  cells  which  have  externally  a  receptive  hairlet  and 
internally  a  contractile  fibre,  and  this  latter  contracts  when  the 
receptive  hairlet  is  stimulated. 

In  view  of  such  cases  it  might  have  seemed  likely  that  in  more 
highly  developed  organisms  examples  would  have  been  forth- 
coming in  which  the  differentiation  of  the  parts  of  a  single  cell 
would  have  advanced  further  still  and  produced  something  yet 
more  akin  to  a  simple  reflex-arc  such  as  is  considered  typical  of 
the  true  nervous  system  itself.  That  expectation  is  not  realized, 
What  we  find  as  the  simplest  arc  in  the  organisms  which  possess 
a  true  nervous  system  is  that  the  conductor  mediating  between 
receptor  and  effector  is  itself  a  separate  cell  intercalated  between 
a  receptive  cell  and  an  effector  cell.  At  each  end  this  separate 
conductive  cell  breaks  up  into  branches.  The  branching  at  the 
receptive  end  places  it  in  communication  not  with  one  but  with 
several  receptor  cells.  This  must  allow  stimuli  at  a  number  of 
receptive  points  to  combine  by  summation  to  a  conjoint  effect. 
By  this  means  the  threshold  of  reaction  will  be  lowered  and  the 
organism  in  that  respect  become  more  sensitively  reactive  to 
the  environment.  At  the  deep,  i.  e.  effector,  end  the  branching 
of  the  conductive  stem  places  it  in  touch  not  with  one  effective 
cell  but  with  many.  Thus,  again,  there  must  result  lowering  of 
the  threshold  —  of  what  we  may  term  the  effective  threshold. 
The  contraction  of  a  single  muscle-fibre  in  a  muscle  is  practi- 
cally ineffective  where  the  resistance  and  mass  of  the  muscle  and 
its  load  are  great  as  compared  with  the  power  of  a  single  muscle- 
fibre.  But  by  its  branching  the  motor  neurone  obtains  hold 
of  many  muscle-fibres.     This  must  tend  to  lower  the  effective 


3IO  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE  BRAIN      [Lect. 

threshold  of  reaction,  and  thus  again  the  organism  is  rendered 
more  delicately  responsive  to  stimulation  by  its  environment. 

But  —  and  it  is  a  striking  fact  —  we  do  not  know  of  any  reflex- 
arc  in  which  in  fact  the  nervous  conductor  connecting  receptor  to 
effector  is  formed  from  end  to  end  of  one  single  neurone.  The 
length  of  the  conductor  seems  always  to  include  at  least  two 
neurones  in  succession.  A  moment's  reflection  reminds  us  that 
such  arrangements  as  Vorticella,  Poteriodendron,  and  the  neuro- 
muscular cells  of  Hydra  and  Actinia  do  not  exhibit  the  germ  of 
a  feature  that  we  have  already  considered  fundamental  in  the 
construction  of  the  reflex  nervous  system.  The  cases  cited  do 
not  exhibit  even  in  germ  the  co-ordinative  mechanism  which  is 
attained  by  the  principle  of  the  common  path.  Such  cases  con- 
fine each  effector  to  the  use  of  one  receptor  only,  and  confine 
each  receptor  to  the  use  of  one  effector  only.  But  we  saw  that 
a  great  principle  in  the  plan  of  the  nervous  system  is  that  an 
effector  shall  be  at  the  behest  of  many  receptors,  and  that  one 
receptor  shall  be  able  to  employ  many  effectors.  We  saw  further 
in  respect  to  this  that  there  are  two  conditions  which  the  ner- 
vous system  satisfies.  One  is  that  the  effector  is  at  the  behest 
of  various  receptors  which  can  use  it  simultaneously  and  use  it 
harmoniously  all  in  more  or  less  the  same  way.  Thus  an  advan- 
tage accrues  in  that  their  reactions  sum,  even  though  the  re- 
ceptors may  be  of  different  modality;  and  by  summation  the 
threshold  is  lowered  and  the  organism  more  sensitized  to  the 
environment.  This  arrangement  cannot  be  obtained  by  the  uni- 
cellular mechanisms  instanced  above.  It  can  only  be  obtained 
by  the  formation  of  a  common  path,  and  the  formation  of  a 
common  path  can  only  be  rendered  possible  by  having  a  con- 
ductor of  pluricellular  length.  And  there  is  another  condition 
which  the  nervous  system  satisfies  The  unicellular  reflex-arc 
—  if  reflex-arc  it  can  be  called —  not  only  admits  no  opportu- 
nity for  pluricellular  summation  but  also  none  for  the  second 
function  of  the  jointed  reflex-arc  of  pluricellular  length,  namely 
"  interference'^  In  animals  of  complex  organization  the  activity 
of  one  effector  organ  may  interfere  with  the  function  of  another, 
e,  g.y  in  the  case  of  muscles  which  when  contracting  pull  in 


IX]  THE   PLURIRECEPTIVE   SUMMATION  311 

opposite  directions  at  the  same  lever.  We  have  seen  how  this 
wasteful  confusion  is  avoided  by  one  receptor  having  power  not 
only  to  throw  a  particular  effector  into  action  but  also  to  throw 
the  opposed  effector  out  of  action.  We  saw  that  this  action  it 
exercises  not  peripherally  but  within  the  nervous  system,  at  the 
entrance  to  a  common  path.  The  unicellular  reflex-arc  allows 
no  common  path.  It  lacks,  therefore,  the  mechanism  which 
renders  possible  the  two  great  co-ordinative  processes  of  pluri- 
receptive  summation  and  of  interference.  Without  these  the 
nervous  system  is  shorn  of  its  chief  powers  to  integrate  a  set 
of  organs  or  an  organism. 

It  is  therefore  a  significant  thing  that  in  the  nervous  system 
there  is  not  only  no  instance  of  the  reflex  triune  —  receptor, 
conductor,  and  effector  —  being  formed  of  one  cell  only,  but 
also  no  indubitable  instance  where  the  middle  link,  the  con- 
ductor, is  even  itself  formed  of  one  cell  (one  neurone)  only.  In 
other  words,  we  know  of  no  instance  in  the  nervous  system  of  a 
reflex-arc  so  constructed  as  not  to  include  a  junction  between 
one  neurone  and  another  neurone.  And  the  rule  is  apparently 
always  that  at  such  junctions  not  only  does  one  neurone  meet 
another,  but  several  neurones  converge  upon  another  and  make 
of  the  latter  a  common  path. 

The  diffuse  nervous  system,  the  gray-centred  nervous  system, 
the  central  nervous  system  a  part  of  the  latter.  The  term  "  nerve- 
centre  "  is  sometimes  abused,  yet  seems  in  several  ways  apt. 
A  keynote  regarding  that  part  of  the  nervous  system  which  is 
termed  "  the  central "  seems  that  it  is  wholly  pieced  together 
into  one  system.  The  nervous  system  in  its  simplest  forms^is\ 
diffuse  —  a  number  of  scattered  mechanisms  performing  merely 
local  operations  with  much  autonomy  save  that  they  have  com- 
munication with  their  immediate  neighbors  across  near  bound- 
aries. The  co-ordination  effected  by  the  diffuse  nervous  system 
is  not  adapted  to  compass  the  quickly  combined  action  of  dis- 
tant parts.  It  is  slow,  and  it  throws  en  route  the  effectors  of 
intermediate  regions  into  action.  It  is  ill  suited,  therefore,  to 
produce  the  integration  of  a  large  and  complex  individual  as 
a  whole,  or  even  to  integrate  large  differentiated  portions  of  an 


312  THE  DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

individual.  Yet  the  co-ordination  it  brings  about  in  its  own 
local  field  may  be  strikingly  effective.  A  co-adjustment  though 
simple  and  restricted  may  be  not  less  perfect  than  one  involving 
wide  and  complex  neural  mechanism.  ]  The  co-ordination  of  a 
peristaltic  movement  of  the  bowel  is,  as  shown  by  Bayliss  and 
Starling,  even  when  managed  exclusively  by  the  local  diffuse 
nervous  system,  capable  of  the  perfect  taxis  of  two  muscular 
coats  arranged  antagonistically  in  the  viscus.  It  directs  a  relaxa- 
tion of  the  one  co-ordinately  with  a  contraction  of  the  other ;  it 
exhibits  a  primitive  but  none  the  less  perfect  form  of  "  recipro- 
cal innervation." 

This  diffuse  system  seems  the  only  one  in  such  an  organism 
as  Medusa.  But  in  higher  animals  a  system  of  longer  direct 
connections  is  developed.  And  this  latter  is  "  synaptic,"  that  is, 
possesses  the  adjustable  junctions  which  belong  characteristically 
to  "  gray  matter."  This  synaptic  system  co-existing  with  the 
diffuse  in  various  places  dominates  the  latter.  Thus  it  controls 
and  overseers  the  actions  of  the  local  nervous  system  of  the  vis- 
cera, and  heart,  and  blood-vessels,  which  even  in  the  highest 
animal  forms  remain  diffuse. 

The  synaptic  nervous  system  has  developed  as  its  distinctive 
feature  a  central  organ,  a  so-called  central  nervous  system  ;  it  is 
through  this  that  it  brings  into  rapport  one  with  another  widely 
distant  organs  of  the  body,  including  the  various  portions  of  the 
diffuse  nervous  system  itself. 

That  portion  of  the  synaptic  system  which  is  termed  "  cen- 
tral "  is  the  portion  where  the  nervous  paths  from  the  various 
peripheral  organs  meet  and  establish  paths  in  common,  i.  e. 
'•'  common  paths y  It  is  therefore  in  accord  with  expectation  that 
we  find  the  organ  in  which  this  meeting  occurs  situated  fairly 
midway  among  them  all,  /.  e.  centrally.  In  bilaterally  symmetri- 
cal animals  this  organ  would  be  expected  to  lie  where  it  does, 
namely,  equidistant  from  the  two  lateral  surfaces  of  the  animal, 
and  to  exhibit  as  it  does,  laterally  symmetrical  halves  united  by 
a  number  of  nervous  cross  ties  bridging  the  median  line.  This 
central  nervous  organ  contains  almost  all  the  junctions  existent 
between  the  multitudinous  conducting  arcs.     In  it  the  afferent 


IX]  THE   DIFFUSE   NERVOUS   SYSTEM  313 

paths  from  receptor-organs  become  connected  with  the  efferent 
paths  of  effector-organs,  not  only  those  adjacent  to  their  own 
receptors  but,  through  ''  intermmciaV  (J.  Hunter,  1778)  paths, 
with  efferent  paths  to  effector-organs  remote.  This  central 
"  exchange  "  organ  is  therefore  well  called  the  central  nervous 
system.  In  the  higher  Invertebrata  it  is  known  as  the  longitu- 
dinal nerve-cord  with  ganglia,  supraoesophageal,  suboesopha- 
geal,  etc. ;  in  Vertebrata  it  is  known  as  the  spinal  cord  and  brain. 
Under  these  different  anatomical  names  the  same  physiological 
organ  is  designated.  It  would  be  more  convenient  for  the  biol- 
ogist were  one  general  term  for  it  in  use.  We  have  seen  that 
it  is  not  merely  a  meeting  place  where  afferent  paths  conjoin 
with  efferent,  but  is,  in  virtue  of  its  physiological  properties, 
an  organ  of  reflex  reinforcements  and  interferences,  and  of 
refractory  phases,  and  shifts  of  connective  pattern ;  that  it 
is,  in  short,  an  organ  of  co-ordination  in  which  from  a  con- 
course of  multitudinous  excitations  there  result  orderly  acts, 
reactions  adapted  to  the  needs  of  the  organism,  and  that  these 
reactions  occur  in  arrangements  {patterns)  marked  by  absence 
of  confusion,  and  proceed  in  sequences  likewise  free  from 
confusion.  "^ — 1 

By  the  development  of  these  powers  the  synaptic  system  » 
with  its  central  organ  is  adapted  to  more  speedy,  wide,  and 
delicate  co-ordinations  than  the  diffuse  nervous  system  allows. 
Out  of  this  potentiality  for  organizing  complex  integration  there 
is  evolved  in  the  synaptic  nervous  system  a  functional  grading 
of  its  reflex  arcs  and  centres.  Thus,  with  allied  reflexes,  the 
mechanism  of  the  common  path  knits  together  by  plurireceptive 
summation  not  only  the  separate  individual  stimuli  of  similar 
kind,  e,g.  tangoreceptive  or  photo  receptive  received  from  some 
agent  as  this  latter  becomes  prepotent  in  the  environment ;  but 
it  knits  together  separate  stimuli  of  even  wholly  different  recep- 
tive species.  C.  J.  Herrick  has  shown  that  in  Ameiurus  nebulo- 
sus  (cat-fish)  the  reaction  of  the  animal  to  stimulation  of  the 
barblets  by  meat  is  a  reaction  to  a  twofold  stimulus,  a  chemical 
and  a  mechanical,  and  he  finds  that  these  two  reactions  mutually 
reinforce.     Nagel  reports  a  similar  case  with  the  tentacle  of  the 


314  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

Actinian,  Aiptasis  saxicola.  v.  Uexkiill  finds  that  the  Gift- 
zangen  of  Echinus  acutiis  react  only  when  a  chemical  and  a 
mechanical  stimulus  are  combined.  The  several  qualitatively 
different  properties  of  an  object  which  is  acting  as  stimulus  are 
thus  combined  and  reinforce  each  other  in  eliciting  appropriate 
reaction.  By  this  summation  reflex  complication  in  Herbart's 
sense  is  made  possible.  A  touchstone  for  rank  of  a  centre  in 
this  neural  hierarchy  is  the  degree  to  which  paths  from  separate 
loci  and  of  different  receptive  modality  are  confluent  thither. 
Indicative  of  high  rank  is  such  functional  position  as  relieves 
from  "  local  work  "  and  involves  general  responsibility,  e.  g.  for 
a  series  of  segments  or  for  the  whole  body.  The  "  three  levels," 
of  Hughlings  Jackson  is  an  expressive  figure  of  this  grading  of 
rank  in  nerve-centres. 

Integrative  action  of  the  nervous  system  in  the  segment  and 
in  the  segmental  series.  In  animal  organisms  of  any  consider- 
able complexity  a  division  of  the  body  into  segments,  metameres, 
is  widely  found.  By  the  occurrence  of  separating  constrictions 
or  sepiments,  or  through  the  regular  repetition  of  appendicular 
structures,  subdivisions  of  the  body  are  established  which  sev- 
erally possess  analogues  of  functions  possessed  more  or  less 
similarly  by  the  other  subdivisions  but  also  severally  possess 
functional  unity.  Such  is  this  functional  unity  and  completeness 
that  in  some  instances  a  metamere  comes  to  be  independent  of 
the  total  organism,  and  able  to  lead  a  separate  existence.  The 
nervous  system  it  is  which  largely  gives  functional  solidarity  to 
the  composite  collection  of  unit  lives  and  organs  composing  the 
individual  metamere.  Further,  the  linkage  of  the  several  meta- 
meres into  one  functional  whole  is  largely  of  nervous  nature. 
The  integrative  function  of  the  nervous  system  is  seen  to  perfec- 
tion in  the  welding  together  of  metameres  into  the  unity  of  an 
animal  individual.  The  kind  of  nervous  system  employed  for 
this  is  the  synaptic  system.  Although  the  nerve-net  system  is 
retained  even  in  the  highest  vertebrates,  it  is  then  confined  to 
unsegmentally  arranged  musculature,  e.g.  visceral  and  vascular. 
In  the  skeletal  musculature,  where  segmental  arrangement  holds, 
the  nervous  system  is  synaptic.     It  is  not  surprising  therefore 


IX]      THE   CENTRALIZED   NERVOUS   SYSTEM       315 

that  in   metameric   animals  the  nervous  system,  especially  its 
synaptic  part,  should  strikingly  exhibit  that  metamerism. 

Various  schemes  of  metamerism  have  been  evolved.  Where 
it  is  radiate  so  that  each  segment  bears  exactly  similar  relations 
to  the  common  axis  and  to  the  other  segments,  the  opportunity 
for  dominance  of  one  segment  over  the  rest  is  slight.  The  con- 
ditions of  life  for  each  segment  are  practically  those  which  are 
the  average  for  all.  The  mouth,  for  instance,  lies  equidistant  from 
them  all.  Evolution  toward  higher  differentiation  of  the  whole 
metameric  individual  and  toward  more  intricate  welding  of  its 
parts  into  one,  is  at  a  disadvantage  in  these  radiate  forms  as 
compared  with  its  opportunity  in  the  great  groups  of  Arthropoda 
and  Vertebrata  where  the  metameres  are  ranged  serially  along  a 
single  axis,  the  longitudinal  axis  of  the  organism.  With  fore 
and  aft  arrangement  of  its  segments  the  animal  body  has  its  first 
opportunity  for  really  high  differentiation.  Certain  of  the  seg- 
ments of  necessity  lie  nearer  to  the  mouth  than  do  others; 
moreover  certain  segments  come  to  habitually  lead,  that  is  to 
say  go  foremost,  during  the  animal's  active  locomotion. 

In  the  integrating  function  of  the  nervous  system  a  segmental 
arrangement  of  its  functions  is  frequently  apparent.  It  makes 
itself  felt  in  two  ways.  Firstly,  the  various  separate  and  differ- 
ent elements  of  the  segment  are  knit  together  by  nervous  ties. 
Secondly,  where  kindred  functions  are  exercised  in  successive 
segments,  so  that  throughout  a  series  of  segments  one  set  of 
organs  forms  a  more  or  less  functionally  homogeneous  system, 
these  organs  are  combined  by  interrelated  nervous  arcs.  But 
particular  systems  of  organs  common  to  all  or  many  metameres 
of  an  individual  present  special  differentiation  of  their  function 
in  particular  metameres.  In  this  manner  the  organism  is  built 
up  of  component  segments  possessing  resemblance  one  to  an- 
other, but  presenting  also  specializations  peculiar  to  certain  seg- 
ments. Hence  the  segmental  arrangement  forms  a  convenient 
basis  not  merely  for  anatomical  but  for  physiological  description. 
And  in  dealing  with  the  special  problems  of  integration  by  the 
nervous  system,  especially  those  of  the  synaptic  nervous  system, 
analysis  can  employ  two  co-ordinate  sets  of  descriptive  factors 


■f 

316  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE   BRAIN      [Lect.  ^ 

—  one  the  segment,  the  other  the  line  of  organs  of  analogous  i 
function  scattered  along  the  series  of  segments.  In  the  two  I 
great  animal  groups  just  mentioned  the  latter  ordinate  is  longi- ^ 
tudinally  extended,  while  the  individual  segment  is  extended  j 
transversely.  The  analysis  thus  proceeds  formally  somewhat  in  ^ 
the  same  way  as  the  analysis  of  a  plane  figure  by  rectangular] 
co-ordinates.  , 

The  receptive  fields.  The  central  nervous  system,  though  | 
divisible  into  separate  mechanisms,  is  yet  one  single  harmoni-l 
ously  acting  although  complex  whole.  To  analyze  its  action  we  ^ 
turn  to  the  receptor-organs,  for  to  them  is  traceable  the  initia- ' 
tion  of  the  reactions  of  the  centres.  These  organs  fall  naturally  , 
into  three  main  groups,  distributed  in  three  main  fields,  each  \ 
field  being  differently  circumstanced. 

Multicellular  animals  regarded  broadly  throughout  a  vast  \ 
range  of  animal  types  are  cellular  masses  presenting  to  the  en-  i 
vironment  a  surface  sheet  of  cells,  and  under  that  a  cellular  , 
bulk  more  or  less  screened  from  the  environment  by  the  surface  j 
sheet.  Many  of  the  agencies  by  which  the  environment  acts  on  | 
the  organism  do  not  penetrate  to  the  deep  cells  inside.  Bedded  \ 
in  the  surface  sheet  are  numbers  of  receptor  cells  constituted  in  1 
adaptation  to  the  stimuli  delivered  by  environmental  agencies.  1 
The  underlying  tissues  devoid  of  these  receptors  are  not  devoid  \ 
of  all  receptor-organs  ;  they  have  other  kinds  apparently  specific  | 
to  them.  Some  agencies  act  not  only  at  the  surface  of  the  ; 
organism  but  penetratively  through  its  mass.  Of  these  there  \ 
are  for  some  apparently  no  receptors  adapted,  for  instance,  none  j 
for  the  Rontgen  rays.  For  others  of  more  usual  occurrence  re-  ■ 
ceptors  are  adapted.  The  most  important  of  these  deep  ade-  ■ 
quate  agents  seems  to  be  mass  acting  in  the  mode  of  weight  and  \ 
mechanical  inertia  involving  mechanical  stresses  and  mechanical 
strain.  Moreover,  the  organism,  like  the  world  surrounding  it,  : 
is  a  field  of  ceaseless  change,  where  internal  energy  is  continu-  \ 
ally  being  liberated,  whence  chemical,  thermal,  mechanical,  and  \ 
electrical  effects  appear.  It  is  a  microcosm  in  which  forces  are  \ 
at  work  as  in  the  macrocosm  around.  In  its  depths  lie  receptor-  \ 
organs  adapted  consonantly  with  the  changes  going  on  in  the  ; 


IX]       METAMERISM   AND   NERVOUS   SYSTEM       317 

microcosm  itself,  particularly  in  its  muscles  and  their  accessory 
apparatus  (tendons,  joints,  walls  of  blood-vessels,  and  the  like). 

There  exist,  therefore,  two  primary  distributions  of  the  recep- 
tor-organs, and  each  constitutes  a  field  in  certain  respects  funda- 
mentally different  from  the  other.  The  deep  field  we  have  called 
ihQ proprio-ceptive  ^oXd,  because  its  stimuli  are,  properly  speaking, 
events  in  the  microcosm  itself,  and  because  that  circumstance 
has  important  bearing  upon  the  service  of  its  receptors  to  the 
organism. 

Richness  of  the  extero-ceptive  field  in  receptors  ;  comparative 
poverty  of  the  intero-ceptive.  The  surface  receptive  field  is 
again  subdivisible.  It  presents  two  divisions.  Of  these  one 
lies  freely  open  to  the  numberless  vicissitudes  and  agencies  of 
the  environment.  That  is  to  say,  it  is  co-extensive  with  the  so- 
called  exterjtal  surface  of  the  animal.  This  subdivision  may  be 
termed  the  extero-ceptive  field. 

But  the  animal  has  another  surface,  its  so-called  internal, 
usually  alimentary  in  function.  This,  though  in  contact  with 
the  environment,  lies  however  less  freely  open  to  it.  It  is 
partly  screened  by  the  organism  itself.  For  purposes  of  retain- 
ing food,  digesting  and  absorbing  it,  an  arrangement  of  common 
occurrence  in  animal  forms  is  that  a  part  of  the  free  surface  is 
deeply  recessed.  In  this  recess  a  fraction  of  the  environment 
is  more  or  less  surrounded  by  the  organism  itself.  Into  that 
sequestered  nook  the  organism  by  appropriate  reactions  gathers 
morsels  of  environmental  material  whence  by  chemical  action 
and  by  absorption  it  draws  nutriment.  This  surface  of  the 
animal  may  be  termed  the  intero-ceptive.  At  its  ingress  several 
species  of  receptors  are  met  with  whose  **  adequate "  stimuli 
are  chemical  {e.  g.  taste  organs).  Lining  this  digestive  chamber, 
this  kitchen,  the  intero-ceptive  surface  is  adapted  to  chemical 
agencies  to  a  degree  such  as  it  exhibits  nowhere  else.  Compara- 
tively little  is  yet  known  of  the  receptor-organs  of  this  surface, 
though  we  may  suppose  that  they  exhibit  refined  adaptations. 
But  the  body-surface  in  this  recess,  though  possessed  of  certain 
receptors  specific  to  it,  is  sparsely  endowed  as  contrasted  with 
that  remainder  of  the  surface  (the  extero-ceptive  surface)  lying 


3i8  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

open  fully  to  the  influences  of  the  great  outer  environment. 
The  afferent  nerve-fibres  in  the  sympathetic  system  as  judged  by 
their  number  in  the  white  rami  are  comparatively  few;  Warring- 
ton's 2^^  recent  observations  show  this  conclusively.  The  poverty 
of  afferent  paths  from  the  intero-ceptive  field  is  broadly  indicated 
by  the  fact  that  we  know  no  wholly  afferent  nerve-trunk  in  the 
sympathetic  (Langley's  "  autonomic  ")  system,  though  such  are 
common  enough  in  the  nervous  system  subserving  the  extero- 
ceptive arcs ;  and  that  in  the  latter  system  we  know  no  wholly 
efferent  nerve-trunk,  whereas  in  the  sympathetic  such  exist,  e.g. 
the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk. 

The  extero-ceptive  field  far  exceeds  the  intero-ceptive  in  its 
wealth  of  receptor-organs.  This  seems  inevitable,  for  it  is  the 
extero-ceptive  surface,  facing  outward  on  the  general  environ- 
ment, that  feels  and  has  felt  for  countless  ages  the  full  stream  of  the 
varied  agencies  forever  pouring  upon  it  from  the  outside  world. 
Mere  enumeration  of  the  different  species  of  receptor-organs 
recognizable  in  it  suffices  to  illustrate  the  importance  of  this 
great  field.  It  contains  specific  receptors  adapted  to  mechanical 
contact,  cold  and  warmth,  light,  sound,  and  agencies  inflicting 
injury  {noxd).  Almost  all  these  species  of  receptors  are  distrib- 
uted to  the  extero-ceptive  field  exclusively ;  they  are  not  known 
to  exist  in  the  intero-ceptive  or  in  the  proprio-ceptive  fields. 

It  is  an  instructive  exercise  to  try  to  classify  the  stimuli  ade- 
quate for  the  receptors  of  the  extero-ceptive  field.  Each  animal 
has  experience  only  of  those  qualities  of  the  environment  which 
as  stimuli  excite  its  receptors ;  it  analyzes  its  environment  in 
terms  of  them  exclusively.  Doubtless  certain  stimuli  causing 
reactions  in  other  animals  are  imperceptible  to  man ;  and  in  a 
large  number  of  cases  his  reactions  are  different  from  theirs. 
Hence  it  is  impossible  for  man  to  conceive  the  world  in  terms 
more  than  partially  equivalent  to  those  of  other  animals.  Hu- 
manly, the  classification  of  adequate  stimuli  can  be  made  with 
various  departments  of  natural  knowledge  as  its  basis.  Physics 
and  chemistry  can  be  taken  as  basis  usefully  in  a  number  of 
cases  where  the  sources  of  stimulation  are  known  to  those  more 
exact  branches  of  experimental  science.     But  in  several  ways  a 


CLASSIFICATION   OF   RECEPTORS  319 

physico-chemical  scheme  of  classification  of  stimuli  lacks  sig- 
nificance for  physiology.  Thus,  in  the  case  of  the  noci-ceptive 
organs  of  the  skin,  those  receptors  —  probably  naked  nerve- 
endings  —  are  non-selective  in  the  meaning  that  they  are  excit- 
able by  physical  and  chemical  stimuli  of  diverse  kind,  radiant, 
mechanical,  acid,  alkaline,  electrical,  and  so  on,  so  that  a  classi- 
fication according  to  mode  of  exciting  energy  on  the  one  hand 
fails  to  differentiate  them  from  each  of  a  number  of  more 
specialized  other  groups  (tango-receptors,  chemo-receptors,  etc.) 
from  which  biologically  they  are  quite  different,  and  on  the 
other  hand  that  classification  apportions  them,  though  physio- 
logically a  single  group,  to  a  whole  series  of  different  classes. 
A  physiological  classification  deals  with  them  more  satisfac- 
torily. Physiological  criteria  can  be  applied  which  at  once 
separate  them  from  other  receptors  and  yet  show  their  affinity 
one  to  another.  Thus,  physiologically  the  stimulus  which  ex- 
cites these  end-organs  must,  whatever  its  physical  or  chemical 
nature,  possess,  in  order  to  stimulate  them,  the  quality  of  tending 
to  do  immediate  harm  to  the  skin.  Further,  the  reflex  they 
excite  (i)  is  prepotent ;  (ii)  tends  to  protect  the  threatened  part 
by  escape  or  defence;  (iii)  is  imperative ;  and  (iv)  if  we  include 
psychical  evidence  and  judge  by  analogy  from  introspection,  is 
accompanied  hy  pain* 

Here  what  we  may  call  the  physiological  scheme  of  classifi- 
cation proves  the  more  useful  at  present.  And  similarly  it  proves 
useful  with  a  group  of  stimuli  that  may  be  termed  "  distance- 
stimuli,"  to  which  we  must  turn  presently.  The  key  to  the 
physiological  classification  lies  in  the  reaction  which  is  produced. 
But  the  physico-chemical  basis  of  classification  also  has  its  uses, 
and  especially  with  those  manifold  receptors  of  the  extero- 
ceptive field  which  possess  highly  developed  accessory  struc- 
tures that  render  them  selectively  receptive  —  and  among  these 
are  some  of  the  most  highly  adapted  and  important  receptors, 
e.g.  photo-receptors,  possessed  by  the  organism.  It  is  to  the 
extero-ceptive  field  that  these  belong. 

Nervous  integration  of  the  segment.  The  edifice  of  the  whole 
central  nervous  system  is  reared  upon  two  neurones,  —  the  affer- 


320  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect.  I 

ent  root-cell  and  the  efferent  root-cell.     These  form  the  pillars  1 

of  a  fundamental  reflex  arch.      And  on  the  junction  between  j 

these  two  are  superposed  and  functionally  set,  mediately  or  im-  j 

mediately,  all  the  other  neural  arcs,  even  those  of  the  cortex  of  I 

the  cerebrum  itself     The  private  receptor  paths  and  the  com-  i 

mon  effector  paths  are  in  the  Chordata  gathered  up  in  a  single  i 

nerve-trunk  for  each  segment.      Close  to  the  central  nervous  \ 

organ,  however,  there  occurs  in  the  segmental  nerves  of  many  | 

Vertebrates  a  cleavage  of  the  private  receptor  paths  from  the  [ 

common  effector  paths.     A  dorsal  spinal  nerve  of  centripetal  I 

conduction   and    a   ventral   of    centrifugal   conduction   results,  i 

Among  the  afferent  root-cells  (afferent  spinal  root  of  Verte-  ' 

brates)    in    each    segment    are    quota    from    the    extero-ceptive  \ 

(cutaneous)   and    from    the  proprio-ceptive  (deep)    fields.      In  \ 

many  segments  there  is  a  third  quotum,  intero-ceptive,  from  the  '^ 
visceral  field.     This  visceral  constituent  of  the  spinal  ganglion  is 

not  present  in  all  segments  and  is  probably,  even  in  those  seg-  ■ 
ments  in  which  it  is  present,  numerically  the   weakest  of  the 

three  components.     Cranial,  caudal,  and  other  segments  exist,  j 

therefore,  in  which  the  total  afferent  nerve  of  the  segment  is  \ 
extero-ceptive  and  proprio-ceptive  but  not  intero-ceptive.     In 

the  remaining  segments  it  is  intero-ceptive  as  well  as  extero-  \ 

ceptive  and  proprio-ceptive,  and  in  these  its  function  is  therefore  i 

fundamentally  threefold.  ! 

The  efferent   segmental  nerve  conversely  radiates   outward 
from  the  central  end  of  the  afferent  nerve  and  the  central  nervous 

organ  to  the  various  effector  organs  at  the  surface  and  in  the  : 

depth   of  the  segment.     Function  does  not,  however,  strictly  ; 

respect   the    ancestral    boundaries   of  segments.      Among  the  ' 
efferent  fibres  in  the  ventral  root   are  a   number   that  extend 
quite  beyond  the  boundaries  of  the  segment  in  which  the  spinal 
root  is  placed.     These  pass  to  the  viscera  and  muscles  of  the 

skin.    They  embouch  not  directly  into  their  effector  organs,  e.g.  ^ 

intestinal  muscle-wall,  pilomotor  muscle,  etc.,  but  into  ganglia  ' 
of  the  sympathetic  system.     In  these  ganglia,  although  not  gray 
matter  in  the  same  sense  as  spinal  cord  and  brain,  axone-endings, 
perikarya,  and  dendrites  are  nevertheless  found.     By  its  distri- 


IX]        NERVOUS   INTEGRATION   OF   SEGMENT       321 

bution  to  the  cells  in  such  a  ganglion  and  by  being  distributed 
in  many  cases  to  more  than  one  such  ganglion,  a  single  con- 
stituent efferent  path  in  the  ventral  spinal  root  obtains  access 
to  a  very  large  number  of  effector  organs.  These  ganglia  seem, 
therefore,  mechanisms  for  the  distribution  of  nerve-impulses. 
We  have  seen  (p.  310)  how  by  such  widening  of  distribution  the 
threshold  of  effective  reaction  is  lowered.  But  though  adapted 
for  distribution  of  nerve-impulses  there  is  no  evidence  that  these 
ganglia  can  serve  for  the  regulation  of  them  in  the  same  sense  as 
does  the  gray  matter  of  the  spinal  cord  with  its  synapses  of 
variable  resistance  and  connection.  Prominent  among  the  inte- 
grating connections  intrinsic  to  each  segment  itself  are  conduct- 
ing paths  from  the  extero-ceptive  field  to  the  *'  final  common 
paths  "  for  the  skeletal  musculature.  Thus,  in  the  mammal  we 
laid  it  down  (Lect.  V,  p.  157)  as  a  general  rule  that  "  for  each 
afferent  root  there  exists  in  immediate  proximity  to  its  own 
place  of  entrance  into  the  cord,  i.  e,  in  its  own  segment,  a  reflex 
motor  path  from  skin  to  muscle  of  as  low  resistance  as  any  open 
to  it  anywhere." 

The  extero-ceptive  arcs  appear  in  most  segments  less  closely 
connected  with  the  visceral  musculature  than  with  the  skeletal 
musculature.  The  intero-ceptive  arcs  appear  in  most  segments 
less  closely  connected  with  the  skeletal  musculature  than  with 
the  visceral.  In  physiological  parlance  a  resistance  to  conduc- 
tion seems  intercalated  between  the  two.  But  both  extero- 
ceptive and  intero-ceptive  fields  easily  influence  through  their 
nervous  arcs  the  musculature  of  the  blood  vascular  organs.  So 
also  do  the  receptors  of  the  proprio-ceptive  field  itself;  and  these 
latter  are  in  particularly  close  touch  with  the  skeletal  muscula- 
ture, exerting  tonic  influence  on  it.  In  certain  segments  these 
general  relations  are  modified  in  special  ways.  Thus,  in  those 
segments  where  the  intero-ceptive  and  extero-ceptive  fields  con- 
join, e.g.  at  the  mouth  and  the  cloaca,  closer  nervous  connec- 
tions exist  between  the  intero-ceptive  arcs  and  the  skeletal 
musculature,  and  conversely  between  the  extero-ceptive  arcs  and 
the  visceral  musculature.  Thus  stimuli  acting  on  the  pharyngeal 
receptors  evoke  or  inhibit  activity  of  skeletal  muscles  subserving 


322  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

respiration  and  deglutition ;  stimuli  to  the  cloacal  mucosa  evoke 
movements  of  the  caudal  skeletal  muscles ;  and  so  forth. 

It  is  not  merely  specific  difference  between  the  receptors  of 
the  extero-ceptive  field  and  those  of  the  intero-ceptive  which 
brings  the  former  into  closer  relationship  with  the  skeletal 
musculature.  Receptors  of  the  one  and  the  same  species,  if 
they  lie  in  the  extero-ceptive  field,  work  skeletal  musculature ; 
if  they  lie  in  the  intero-ceptive,  work  visceral  musculature. 
Thus  the  chemo-receptors  on  the  outer  surface  of  the  head 
(gustatory  of  the  barblets  of  fish)  excite  reflexes  which  move 
the  body  round,  bringing  the  mouth  to  the  morsel ;  while  the 
similar  chemo-receptors  within  the  mouth  excite  reflex  swallow- 
ing without  outward  movement  of  the  animal  (C.  J.  Herrick). 

Receptors  of  the  same  specific  system,  where  they  lie  close 
together,  mutually  reinforce  reaction.  On  the  contrary,  where 
members  of  two  different  systems  lie  close  together,  e.g.  tango- 
receptor  and  noci-ceptor,  in  one  and  the  same  piece  of  skin,  they, 
as  mentioned  above,  often  have  conflicting  mutual  relation. 
One  relationship  between  receptor  arcs  of  the  same  species 
may  be  particularly  noted.  Receptors  symmetrically  placed  on 
opposite  sides  of  the  segment,  especially  if  distant  from  the 
median  plane,  excite  reactions  which  mutually  **  conflict."  Thus, 
when  a  noci-ceptor  is  stimulated  on  the  right  side  of  the  tail  of 
the  spinal  dog  or  cat  or  lizard,  the  reaction  moves  the  organ  to 
the  left.  The  symmetrical  receptor  on  the  left  side  does  the 
converse.  The  two  reactions  thus  conflict.  And  the  like  holds 
true  for  the  many  right  and  left  symmetrical  receptors  which 
initiate  exactly  converse  reactions. 

But  a  group  of  special  cases  is  formed  by  reactions  initiated 
from  receptors  distributed  at  or  near  to  the  median  line.  Stimu- 
lation of  such  a  small  group  of  receptors  at  the  median  line  in 
many  cases  evokes  a  bilateral  movement  which  is  symmetri- 
cal, e.  g.  a  touch  on  the  decerebrate  frog's  lip  in  the  median 
line  causes  both  fore  limbs  to  sweep  forward  synchronously 
over  the  spot.  The  median  overlap  of  the  distribution  of  the 
afferent  fibres  of  the  dorsal  spinal  roots  may  be  connected  with 
this. 


REFINEMENTS   OF   RECEPTORS  323 


I 

^B  Special  refinements  of  the  receptors  of  the  "leading  "  segments. 
As  the  receptors  that  are  excitable  by  the  various  adequate 
agencies,  e.g.  mechanical  impact,  noxa,  radiant  energy,  chemical 
solutions,  etc.,  are  traced  along  the  series  of  segments,  it  is  found 
that  in  one  region  of  the  longitudinal  segmental  series  remark- 
able developments  exist. 

In  motile  animals  constituted  of  segments  ranged  along  a 
single  axis,  e.g.  Vertebrata,  when  locomotion  of  the  animal  goes 
on,  it  proceeds  for  the  most  part  along  a  line  continuous  with 
the  long  axis  of  the  animal  itself,  and  more  frequently  in  one 
direction  of  that  line  than  in  the  other.  The  animal's  loco- 
motor appendages  and  their  musculature  are  favorably  adapted 
for  locomotion  in  that  habitual  direction.  In  the  animal's  pro^7 
gression  certain  of  its  segments  therefore  lead.  The  receptors 
of  these  leading  segments  predominate  in  the  motor  taxis  of  the 
animal.  They  are  specially  developed.  Thus,  in  the  earth- 
worm, while  all  parts  of  the  external  surface  are  responsive  to 
light,  the  directive  influence  of  light  is  greatest  at  the  anterior 
end  of  the  animal.  The  leading  segments  are  exposed  to  exter- 
nal influences  more  than  are  the  rest.  Not  only  do  they  receive 
more  stimuli,  meet  more  "  objects  "  demanding  pursuit  or  avoid- 
ance, but  it  is  they  which  usually  first  encounter  the  agents 
beneficial  or  hurtful  of  the  environment  as  related  to  the  indi- 
vidual. Pre-eminent  advantage  accrues  if  the  receptors  of  these 
leading  segments  react  sensitively  and  differentially  to  the  agen- 
cies of  the  environmentj  And  it  is  in  these  leading  segments 
that  remarkable  developments  of  the  receptors,  especially  those 
of  the  extero-ceptive  field,  arise.  Some  of  them  are  specialized 
in  such  degree  as  almost  obscures  their  fundamental  affinity  to 
others  distributed  in  other  segments.  Thus,  among  the  system 
of  receptors  for  which  radiation  is  the  adequate  agent,  there 
are  developed  in  one  of  the  leading  segments  a  certain  group, 
the  retinal,  particularly  and  solely,  and  extraordinarily  highly, 
amenable  to  radiations  of  a  certain  limited  range  of  wave-length. 
These  are  t\iQ photo-receptors,  for  which  light  and  only  light,  e.g. 
not  heat,  is  the  adequate  stimulus.  In  like  manner  a  certain 
group  belonging  to  the  system  receptive  of  mechanical  impacts 


324  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE  BRAIN       [Lect. 

attains  such  susceptibility  for  these  as  to  react  to  the  vibrations 
of  water  and  air  that  constitute  physical  sounds.  The  retina  is 
thus  a  group  of  glorified  "  warm-spots,"  the  cochlea  a  group  of 
glorified  "  touch-spots."  Again,  a  group  belonging  to  the  sys- 
tem adapted  to  chemical  stimuH  reach  in  one  of  the  leading 
segments  such  a  pitch  of  delicacy  that  particles  in  quantity  un- 
weighable  by  the  chemist,  emanating  from  substances  called 
odorous,  excite  reaction  from  them. 

The  refined  receptors  of  the  leading  segments  are  "  distance- 
receptors."  The  after-coming  segments  form  a  motor  train  actuated 
chiefly  by  the  "  distance -receptors."  It  is  in  the  leading  segments 
that  we  find  the  "  distance-receptors,"  For  so  may  be  called  the 
receptors  which  react  to  objects  at  a  distance.  These  are  the 
same  receptors  which,  acting  as  sense-organs,  initiate  sensations 
having  the  psychical  quality  termed  projicieiice.  The  receptor- 
organs  adapted  to  odors,  light,  and  sound,  though  stimulated 
by  the  external  matter  in  direct  contact  with  them,  —  as  the 
vibrating  ether,  the  vibrating  water  or  air,  or  odorous  particles, 
—  yet  generate  reactions  which  show  **  adaptation,"  e.g.  in 
direction  of  movements,  etc.,  to  the  environmental  objects  at  a 
distance,  the  sources  of  those  changes  impinging  on  and  acting 
as  stimuli  at  the  organism's  surface.  We  know  that  in  ourselves 
sensations  initiated  through  these  receptors  are  forthwith  '*  pro- 
jected "  into  the  world  outside  the  "  material  me."  The  proji- 
cience  refers  them,  without  elaboration  by  any  reasoned  mental 
process,  to  directions  and  distances  in  the  environment  fairly 
accurately  corresponding  with  the  "  real  "  directions  and  dis- 
tances of  their  actual  sources.  None  of  the  sensations  initiated 
in  the  proprio-ceptive  or  intero-ceptive  fields  possess  this  prop- 
erty of  projicience.  And  with  the  distance-receptors  considered 
simply  as  originators  of  reflex  actions,  their  reflexes  are  found 
to  be  appropriate  to  the  stimuli  as  regards  the  direction  and 
distance  of  the  sources  of  these  latter.  Thus,  the  patch  of 
light  constituting  a  retinal  image  excites  a  reflex  movement 
which  turns  the  eyeball  toward  the  source  of  the  image  and 
adjusts  ocular  accommodation  to  the  distance  of  that  source 
from  the  animal  itself.     Even  a  negative  stimulus  suffices.     The 


IX]  DISTANCE   RECEPTORS  325 

shadow  of  the  hand  put  out  to  seize  the  tortoise  excites,  as  it 
blots  the  retinal  illumination,  withdrawal  of  the  animal's  head  to 
within  the  shelter  of  the  shell. 

How  this  result  of  **  distance  "  has  been  acquired  is  hard  to  say. 
The  net  effect  is  reached  in  various  ways,  and  with  very  various 
gain  in  the  degree  of  *'  distance  "  acquired.  By  long  vibrissae 
certain  tango-receptors  obtain  excitation  from  objects  still  at  a 
distance  from  the  general  surface  of  the  organism.  By  reduc- 
tion of  their  threshold  value  of  stimulus,  certain  other  receptors 
akin  to  tactual,  inasmuch  as  their  adequate  stimuli  are  mechan- 
ical, become  responsive  to  vibratory  movements  of  water  and  air 
so  as  to  react  to  physical  sounds  whose  sources  lie  remote  from 
the  animal.  Certain  chemo-receptors  acquire  so  low  a  threshold 
that  they  react  not  merely  to  food  and  other  substances  in  con- 
tact with  them  in  mass,  but  react  to  almost  inconceivably  diluted 
traces  of  such,  traces  which  drift  off  from  the  objects  and  per- 
meate the  environment  through  long  distances,  as  so-called 
odors,  before  impinging  upon  the  delicate  receptors  in  ques- 
tion. The  leading  segments  thus  come  to  possess  not  only 
taste,  but  taste  at  a  distance,  namely  smell.  In  such  cases  it 
ieems  chiefly  by  lowering  of  their  threshold  that  these  receptors 
»f  the  leading  segments  have  been  brought  to  react  to  objects 
till  remote  from  the  organism. 

The  "  distance-receptors  "  seem  to  have  peculiar  importance 
►r  the  construction  and  evolution  of  the  nervous  system.  In 
le  higher  grades  of  the  animal  scale  one  part  of  the  nervous  sys- 
\m  has,  as  Gaskell  insists,  evolved  with  singular  constancy  a 
(ominant  importance  to  the  individual.  That  is  the  part  which 
called  the  brain.  T/ie  brain  is  always  the  part  of  the  nervous 
fstem  which  is  constructed  upon  and  evolved  upon  the  "  distance- 
\eceptor''  organs.  Their  effector  reactions  and  sensations  are 
idently  of  paramount  importance  in  the  functioning  of  the 
lervous  system  and  of  the  individual.  This  seems  explicable, 
^t  least  partly,  in  the  following  manner. 

An  animal  organism  is  not  a  machine  which  merely  trans- 
>rms  a  quantum  of  energy  given  it  in  potential  form  at  the  out- 
;t  of  its  career.     It  has  to  replenish  its  potential  energy  by 


326  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE  BRAIN      [Lect. 

continued  acquisition  of  suitable  energy-containing  material  from 
the  environment,  and  this  material  it  has  to  incorporate  in  itself. 
Moreover,  since  death  cuts  short  the  career  of  the  individual 
organism,  the  species  has  to  be  maintained,  and  for  that  in  most 
higher  organisms  there  is  required  accession  of  material  (ga- 
metic) from  another  organism  (of  like  species)  to  rejuvenesce 
a  portion  of  the  adult,  which  portion  then  cast  off  leads  a  new 
individual  existence.  To  satisfy,  therefore,  the  primary  vital  re- 
quirements of  an  animal  species,  actual  material  contact  with 
certain  objects  is  necessary;  thus,  for  feeding,  and  in  many 
cases  for  sexual  reproduction. 

In  these  processes  of  feeding  and  conjugation  the  non- 
distance-receptors  play  an  important  and  essential  part.  But 
ability  on  the  part  of  an  organism  to  react  to  an  object  when 
still  distant  from  it  allows  an  interval  for  preparatory  reactive 
steps  which  can  go  far  to  influence  the  success  of  attempt  either 
to  obtain  actual  contact  or  to  avoid  actual  contact  with  the 
object.  Thus,  we  may  take  in  illustration  the  two  sets  of  selec- 
tive chemo-receptors,  the  gustatory  and  the  olfactory.  Both  are 
responsive  to  certain  chemical  stimuli  which  reach  them  through 
solution  in  the  moist  mucous  membranes  of  the  mouth  and  nose. 
No  odorous  substance  appears  to  be  tasteless,  and  if  the  thresh- 
old value  for  olfaction  and  for  taste  be  measured  respectively, 
the  threshold  for  the  former  as  determined  in  weight  of  dissolved 
material  is  lower  than  for  the  latter.  The  former  is  the  distance- 
receptor.  Animal  behavior  shows  clearly  that  in  regard  to 
these  two  groups  of  receptors  the  one  subserves  differentiation 
of  reaction,  i.  e.  swallowing  or  rejection,  of  material  already 
found  and  acquired,  e.g.  within  the  mouth.  The  other,  the 
distance-receptor,  smell,  initiates  and  subserves  far-reaching 
complex  reactions  of  the  animal  anticipatory  to  swallowing, 
namely,  all  that  train  of  reaction  which  may  be  comprehensively 
termed  the  quest  for  food.  The  latter  foreruns  and  leads  up  to 
the  former.  This  precurrent  relation  of  the  reaction  of  the 
distance-receptor  to  the  non-distance  receptor  is  typical. 

The  "distance-receptors"  initiate  anticipatory,  i.  e.  precurrent, 
reactions.    I  ventured  above  to  use  the  word  "  attempt."    Just  as 


IX]  DISTANCE   RECEPTORS  327 

salient  character  of  most  of  the  reactions  of  the  non-projicient 
eceptors  taken  as  sense-organs  is  "  affective  tone"  i, e.  physical 
ain  or  physical  pleasure,  so  "  conative  feeling "  is  salient  as  a 
sychical  character  of  the  reactions  which  the  projicient  or 
istance-receptors,  taken  as  sense-organs,  guide.  As  initiators 
f  reflex  movements  the  action  of  these  latter  is  characterized 
y  tendency  to  work  or  control  the  musculature  of  the  animal 
a  whole y  —  as  a  single  machine,  —  to  impel  locomotion  or  to 
ut  it  short  by  the  assumption  of  some  total  posture,  some  atti- 
de  which  involves  steady  posture  not  of  one  limb  or  one 
ppendage  alone,  but  of  all,  so  as  to  maintain  an  attitude  of  the 
ody  as  a  whole.  Take,  for  instance,  the  flight  of  a  moth  toward 
candle,  the  dash  of  a  pike  toward  a  minnow,  and  the  tense 
iteadiness  of  a  frog  about  to  seize  an  insect.  These  reactions 
re  all  of  them  excited  by  distance-receptors.  Though  in  the 
ne  case  the  musculature  is  impelled  to  locomotion  toward  the 
stimulus  (positive  phototropism),  in  the  other  restrained  (in- 
hibited) from  locomotion.  Whether  the  reaction  be  move- 
ment toward  or  movement  away  from  (positive  or  negative)  or 
Khether  it  be  motion  or  its  restraint  (excito-motor  or  inhibito- 
lotor)  does  not  matter  here.  The  point  here  is  that  in  both  re- 
:tions  the  skeletal  musculature  is  treated  practically  as  a  whole 
and  in  a  manner  suitably  anticipatory  of  a  later  event.  That  is 
far  less  the  case  with  the  non-projicient  receptors.  The^decere;^ 
^Jh:^  frog  changes  the  whole  direction  of  its  path  of  locomotion 
when  a  visual  obstacle  is  set  in  its  way,  but  a  skin  impact  ex- 
cites a  movement  in  a  small  field  of  musculature  only,  e,  g.  the 
eyelidblinks  oncorneal  contact,  the  foot  flexes  at  a  digital  noxa; 
where  the  part  itself  cannot  well  move  itself  musculature  acces- 
sory to  it  but  distant  from  it  is  moved.  Thus  the  hind  limb  is 
swept  over  the  flank  on  irritation  there,  or  the  fore  limb  over 
the  snout  on  irritation  there.  But  in  these  cases  the  movement 
induced  is  merely  local  and  does  not  affect  the  body  as  a  whole. 
Sufficient  intensity  (we  may  include  summation  under  intensity) 

I\{  a  stimulus  can  of  course  impel  the  whole  creature  to  move- 
nent  even  through  a  non-projicient  receptor.    A  decerebrate  frog 


328  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

touch,  and  again  more  so  at  a  second ;  at  a  third  will,  besides 
lowering  the  head,  draw  the  front  half  of  its  trunk  slightly 
backward;  at  a  fourth  the  same  movement  with  stronger  re- 
traction ;  at  a  fifth  give  an  ineffectual  sweep  with  its  hind  or 
fore  foot;  at  a  sixth  a  stronger  sweep;  at  a  seventh  a  feeble 
jump;  at  an  eighth  a  free  jump,  and  so  forth.  Considerable 
intensity  or  summation  is  required  to  evoke  a  reflex  reaction 
of  the  skeletal  musculature  as  a  whole  from  these  cutaneous 
receptors.  The  projicient  receptors  and  their  reflexes  once 
gone,  even  intense  stimuli  do  not  readily  move  or  arrest  the 
creature  as  a  whole.  It  is  relatively  difficult  to  get  the  "  spinal " 
frog  to  spring  or  swim.  Co-ordinate  movement  of  the  crea- 
ture as  a  whole  is  then  obtained  by  general  stimulation  {i.  e. 
plurireceptive  summation),  or  if  by  localized  stimulation  the 
stimulus  must  be  intense.  Thus  the  spinal  frog  will  swim 
when  placed  in  water  at  36°  C.  The  warm  water  forms  a  noci- 
ceptive stimulus  to  the  receptors  of  the  immersed  body-surface 
generally. 

Extensive  internuncial  paths  of  **  distance-receptors."  Con- 
formably with  the  power  of  the  "  distance-receptors  "  to  induce 
movements  or  postures  of  the  individual  as  a  whole  we  find  the 
neural  arcs  from  these  receptors  particularly  wide  and  far-reach- 
ing. The  nerve-fibre  that  starts  from  the  receptor  does  not  in 
many  of  these  cases  itself  extend  to,  or  send  processes  to,  the 
mouths  of  the  ^'' final  common  paths''  Instead  of  doing  so  it 
ends  often  far  short  of  them,  and  forms  connection  with  other 
nerve-fibres  (internuncial  paths),  which  in  their  turn  reach  dis- 
tant "  final  common  paths."  This  arrangement  involves  an  inter- 
calation of  gray  matter  between  the  **  private  receptor  "  path  and 
the  "  final  common  path  "  not  only  at  the  mouth  of  the  latter,  but 
also  where  the  internuncial  path  itself  commences.  The  signifi- 
ca?tce  of  this  seems  that  the  internuncial  path  is  itself  a  **  common 
path,  and  therefore  a  mechanism  of  accommodation''  Its  com- 
munity of  function  is  not  so  extensive  as  that  of  a  "  final  com- 
mon path,"  not  co-extensive  for  instance  with  all  the  receptors 
of  the  body,  as  would  appear  the  case  with  a  motor-nerve  to  a 
skeletal  muscle.     Yet  it  furnishes  a  path  for  use  by  certain  sets 


PRECURRENT   REACTIONS  329 


I 

^■f  receptors  in  common.     In  Mustelus  the  nerve  paths  from  the 
^Betinal  and  from  the  olfactory  receptors  converge  toward  the  roof- 
^Bucleus  of  the  mid-brain,  whence  passes  the  long  mesencephalo- 
^^pinal  path  to  the  spinal  motor  nuclei.     The  inference  is  that 
conjoint  stimulation  of  eye  and  nose  exert  a  combined  influence 
I^Kid  impinge  together  on  the  spinal  motor  machinery.    Similarly 
^^le  Reissner  fibre  ^^^  may  serve  as  an  internuncial  path  between 
^paths  coming  in  from  olfactory  and  visual  receptors  on  the  one 
^Hand  and  the  spinal  motor  common  paths  from  the  spinal  cord 
^H>  the  muscles  on  the  other.     Another  instance  of  an  internun- 
^Kal  path  is  the  so-called  "  pyramidal  tract "  characteristic  of  the 
^Hiammalian  nervous  system.     It  furnishes  a  path  of  internuncial 
character  common  to  certain  arcs  that  have  arisen  indirectly 
from   various  receptors  of  various  species  and   are  knitted  to- 
gether in  the  cerebral   hemisphere.      Another  instance  is  the 
path  from  the  thalamus  to  the  post-central  convolution  (Mott, 
Tschermak,  and  others). 

Precurrent  reactions.  Consummatory  reactions.  It  might 
seem  at  first  that  all  motor  reflexes  may  be  grouped  into  those 
that  tend  to  prolong  the  stimulus  and  those  that  tend  to  cut 
it  short.  Consideration  shows  that  such  a  grouping  expresses 
I^Wie  truth  but  partially.  We  argued  above  that  the  **  distance- 
^^i"eceptors  "  induce  anticipatory  or  precurrent  reactions,  that  is, 
precurrent  to  final  or  consummatory  reactions.  The  reflexes  of 
certain  non-projicient  receptors  stand  in  very  close  relation  to 
"  consummatory"  events.  Thus  the  tango-receptors  of  the  lips 
and  mouth  initiate  reflex  movements  that  immediately  precede 
the  act  which  for  the  individual  creature  viewed  as  a  conative 
afid  a  sentient  agent  is  the  final  consummatory  one  in  respect  to 
nutriment  as  a  stimulus,  namely,  swallowing.  Similarly  with  the 
gustato-receptors  and  their  reactions.  The  sequence  of  action 
initiated  by  these  non-projicient  receptors  is  a  short  one :  their 
reflex  leads  immediately  to  another  which  is  consummatory. 
Those  receptors  of  the  chelae  of  Astacus,  Homarus,  etc.,  which 
initiate  the  carrying  of  objects  to  the  mouth,  or  again  the  tango- 

Ieceptors  of  the  hand  of  the  monkey  when  it  plucks  fruit  and 
arries  it  to  the  lips,  give  reactions  a  step  further  from  the  con- 
I 


330  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE  BRAIN       [Lect. 

summatory  than  those  just  instanced.  These  reactions  are  all 
steps  toward  final  adjustments^  and  are  not  themselves  end-points. 
The  series  of  actions  of  which  the  distance-receptors  initiate  the 
earlier  steps  form  series  much  longer  than  those  initiated  by  the 
non-projicient.  Their  stages,  moreover,  continue  to  be  guided 
by  the  projicient  organs  for  a  longer  period  between  initiation 
and  consummation.  Thus  in  a  positive  phototropic  reaction  the 
eye  continues  to  be  the  starting  place  of  the  excitation,  and 
in  many  cases  guides  change  in  the  direction  not  only  of  the 
eyeball  but  of  the  whole  animal  in  locomotion  as  the  reflex 
proceeds.  The  mere  length  of  their  series  of  steps  and  the  vicis- 
situdes of  relation  between  bodies  in  motion  reacting  on  one 
another  at  a  distance  conspire  to  give  to  these  precurrent  re- 
flexes a  multiformity  and  complexity  unparalleled  by  the  reflexes 
from  the  non-projicient  receptors.  The  reaction  started  by 
**  distance-receptors  "  where  positive  not  only  leads  up  to  the 
consummatory  reactions  of  the  non-projicient,  but  on  the  way 
thither  associates  with  it  stimulation  of  other  projicient  recep- 
tors, as  when,  for  instance,  a  phototropic  reaction  on  the  part  of 
a  Selachian  brings  the  olfactory  organs  into  range  of  an  odorous 
prey,  or,  conversely,  when  the  beagle  sees  the  hare  after  run- 
ning it  by  scent.  In  such  a  case  the  visual  and  olfactory  recep- 
tor arcs  would  be  related  as  "  allied  "  arcs  (Lecture  IV),  and 
reinforce  each  other  in  regard  to  the  mesencephalo-spinal  path, 
or  in  higher  mammals  the  **  pyramidal "  or  other  pallio-spinal 
path.  It  is  easy  to  see  what  copious  opportunity  for  adjust- 
ment and  of  side  connection  such  a  reaction  demands,  consist- 
ing as  it  does  of  a  number  of  events  in  serial  chain,  each  link  a 
modification  of  its  predecessor. 

Strong  affective  tone  an  accompaniment  of  consummatory  re- 
actions. We  may  venture  to  turn  briefly  to  the  psychical  aspect 
of  such  sequences.  To  consummatory  reactions  affective  tone 
seems  adjunct  much  more  than  to  the  anticipatory,  especially  the 
remotely  anticipatory  of  the  projicient  sense-organs.  Thus  the 
affective  tone  of  *'  tastes  "  is  strong.  The  reaction  initiated  by  a 
noci-ceptor  (pp.  226-23 1)  is  to  be  regarded  as  consummatory.  The 
application  of  an  irritant  to  the  flank  of  a  frog  evokes  a  movement 


AFFECTIVE   TONE  331 

f  the  leg  adapted  to  at  once  remove  that  stimulus  from  the  skin 
f  the  flank.  Or  again,  an  irritant  applied  to  the  skin  of  the 
ot  evokes  a  movement  of  the  foot  away  from  that  stimulus, 
both  cases  the  reaction  is  a  consummatory  one,  because  it  is 
Iculated  of  itself  to  be  final.  To  judge  by  our  own  introspec- 
on  the  affective  tone  adjunct  to  these  reactions  is  strong.  They 
stance  strong  affective  tone  pertaining  to  consummatory  re- 
tions.  The  affective  tone  of  the  reactions  of  the  projicient 
ceptors  is  less  marked :  physical  pleasure  or  pain  can  hardly 
said  to  accompany  them.  Not  of  course  that  they  are  wholly 
nrelated  to  affective  tone.  The  relative  haste  with  which  an 
imal  when  hungry  approaches  food  offered  to  the  visual  field 
suggests  that  conation  attaches  to  the  visual  reaction  by  asso- 
ciation through  memory  with  affective  tone.  By  associative 
memory  a  tinge  of  the  affective  tone  of  the  consummatory  reac- 
tion may  suffuse  the  anticipatory.  The  latter  becomes  indirectly 
a  pleasure-pain  reaction.  The  neutral  tango-receptive  reactions 
of  the  feet  of  the  tortoise  hastening  stumblingly  towards  its  food 
may  in  this  way  be  imbued  with  a  tinge  of  affective  tone  derived 

irom  the  affective  tint  of  the  leading  reflex,  namely  the  visual, 
i^hich  itself  has  thus  memorial  association  with  a  consummatory 
eflex  of  strong  affective  tone.    Examples  of  this  type  of  reaction 
urnished   by  new-born  animals  are  given  by  Lloyd  Morgan.^®* 
When  "  after  a  few  days  the  new-born  chick  leaves  ladybirds 
mmolested  while  he  seizes  wasp-larvae  with  increased  energy  '* 
ne  affords  evidence  that  reactions  of  his  projicient  receptors 
have  acquired  a  new  value,  and  that  value  is  made  up  mediately 
f  affective  tone.      How  they  have  acquired  it  or  what  exact 
ature  their  new  attribute  has  is  not  our  question.     It  is  enough 
ere  that  in  regard  to  certain  stimuli  the  new  value  —  the  mean- 
g  —  which  the  projicient  sensation  has  obtained  has  reinforced 
eatly  the  conative  intensity  of  the  reaction  to  the  stimulus.    It 
as  given  the  stimulus  increased  force  as  a  spring  of  precurrent 
ctions  aimed  at  a  final  consummatory  one.    It  has  given  this  not 
y  altering  the  external  stimulus,  nor  the  receptor-organ,  but  by, 
mong  other  alterations,  altering  internal  connections  of  the  re- 
ptor  arc.    Thus  it  is  that,  be  it  by  associative  memory  or  other 


332  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

processes,  the  reactions  of  the  "  distance-receptors  "  come  in 
higher  animals  to  reveal  a  conative  driving  force  which  is  per- 
haps the  end  for  which  these  psychoses  exist. 

Nor  are  the  series  of  reactions,  short  though  they  be,  which 
the  non-projicient  receptors  initiate  wholly  devoid  of  conative 
appearance.  They  show  adaptation  as  executive  of  steps  toward 
an  end.  Food,  sexual  consummation,  suitable  posture,  preser- 
vation from  injury,  are  ends  to  which  their  direction  leads,  as 
with  the  longer  series  of  actions  due  to  projicient  receptors 
reacting  to  objects  at  a  wider  horizon.  It  is  rather  that  the  lat- 
ter afford  a  freer  field  for  the  winning  more  subtle  adjustments 
with  wider  application  of  associative  memory.  In  the  latter 
there  is  more  scope  for  the  play  of  mind,  —  mind  it  may  be  of 
such  elementary  grade  as  to  be  difficult  for  us  to  picture  in  its 
operations. 

We  may  suppose  that  in  the  time  run  through  by  a  course 
of  action  focussed  upon  a  final  consummatory  event,  oppor- 
tunity is  given  for  instinct,  with  its  germ  of  memory  however 
rudimentary  and  its  germ  of  anticipation  however  slight,  to 
evolve  under  selection  that  mental  extension  of  the  present 
backward  into  the  past  and  forward  into  the  future  which  in  the 
highest  animals  forms  the  prerogative  of  more  developed  mind. 
Nothing,  it  would  seem,  could  better  ensure  the  course  of  action 
taken  in  that  interval  being  the  right  one  than  memory  and 
anticipatory  forecast :  and  nothing,  it  would  seem,  could  tend  to 
select  more  potently  the  individuals  taking  the  right  course  than 
the  success  which  crowns  that  course,  since  the  consummatory 
acts  led  up  to  are  such  —  e.g.  the  seizure  of  prey,  escape  from 
enemies,  attainment  of  sexual  conjugation,  etc.  —  as  involve  the 
very  existence  of  the  individual  and  the  species.  The  problem 
before  the  lowlier  organism  is  in  some  slight  measure  shadowed 
to  us  by  the  difficulties  of  adjustment  of  reaction  shown  by  the 
human  child.  The  child,  although  his  reactions  are  perfect 
within  a  certain  sphere  of  his  surroundings,  shows  himself  at  the 
confines  of  that  sphere  a  little  blunderer  in  a  world  of  over- 
whelming meaning.  Hence  indeed  half  the  pathos  and  humour 
derivable  from  childhood. 


RECEPTIVE    RANGE  333 


I 

I^P  It  is  the  long  serial  reactions  of  the  "  distance-receptors  " 
that  allow  most  scope  for  the  selection  of  those  brute  organisms 
that  are  fittest  for  survival  in  respect  to  elements  of  mind.  The 
'•  distance-receptors  "  hence  contribute  most  to  the  upr earing  of  the 
v^K^ednim,  Swallowing  was  above  termed  a  consummatory  reac- 
^^^on.  Once  through  the  maw,  the  morsel  is,  we  know  by 
introspection,  under  normal  circumstances  lost  for  conscious- 
ness. But  it  nevertheless  continues  to  excite  receptors  and 
their  nervous  arcs.  The  significant  point  is  that  the  object  has 
passed  into  such  a  relation  with  the  surface  of  the  organism 
that  "  conation  "  is  no  longer  of  advantage.  The  naive  notion 
that  when  we  have  eaten  and  drunken  we  have  fed  is  justified 
practically.  No  effort  can  help  us  to  incorporate  the  food 
further.  Conation  has  then  done  its  all  and  has  no  further 
utility  in  respect  to  that  food  taken.  It  is  significant  that  all 
direct  psychical  accompaniment  of  the  reactions  ceases  abruptly 
at  this  very  point.  The  immediately  precedent  reactions  that 
were  psychically  suffused  with  strong  affective  colour  pass  ab- 
ruptly over  into  reactions  not  merely  affectively  neutral  but  void 
—  normally  —  of  psychical  existence  altogether.  The  concomi- 
tance between  certain  nervous  reactions  and  psychosis  seems  an 
alliance  that  strengthens  the  restless  striving  of  the  individual 
animal  which  is  the  passport  of  its  species  to  continuance  of 
existence. 

Receptive  range.  The  ascendency  of  "distance-receptors  "  in 
the  organization  of  neural  function  may  be  partly  traceable  to  the 
T^XdXxwt  frequency  of  their  use.  Although  it  would  be  incorrect 
to  assess  the  value  of  an  organ  by  the  mere  frequency  with  which 
it  is  of  service,  yet  caeteris  paribus  that  seems  a  fair  criterion. 
The  frequency  with  which  a  receptor  meets  its  stimuli  is,  other 
things  being  equal,  proportionate  to  the  size  of  the  slice  of  the 
external  world  which  lies  within  its  "  receptive  ranged  Although 
in  a  fish,  for  instance,  the  skin  with  its  tango-receptors  is  much 
larger  in  area  than  are  the  retinae  with  their  photo-receptors,  the 
restricted  "  receptive-range" — the  adequate  stimulus  requiring 
actual  proximity  —  of  the  former  gives  a  far  smaller  slice  of  the 

imulus-containing  world  to  the  skin  than  pertains  to  the  eyes. 


334  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

In  the  case  of  the  eye  not  only  is  the  slice  of  environment  pertain- 
ing to  it  at  even  a  short  distance  more  wide  and  high  than  that  of 
the  skin,  but  it  is  at  each  moment  multiplied  by  the  third  dimen- 
sion. There  arise  in  it,  therefore  {caeteris paribus) ^  in  unit  of  time 
many  more  stimulations,  with  the  result  that  the  receptor-organ 
of  "  distant"  species  receives  many  more  fresh  stimuli  per  unit  of 
time  than  does  the  receptor-organ  of  restricted  receptive  range. 
The  greater  richness  of  the  neural  construction  of  the  photo- 
receptive  system  than  of  \he  tango-receptive  accords  with  this. 
Thus  in  the  photo- receptive  system  the  so-called  '*  optic  nerve  " 
(which  since  it  is  the  second  neural  link  and  therefore  to  some 
extent  a  "  common  path,"  presents  numerical  reduction  from  the 
first  or  private  path  in  the  retina  itself)  contains  more  conduc- 
tive channels  (nerve-fibres)  in  man  (i, 000,000,  Krause)  than  are 
contained  in  the  whole  series  of  afferent  spinal  roots  of  one  side 
of  the  body  put  together  (634,000,  Ingbert  2^*»  ^^),  and  of  these 
latter  the  cutaneous  afferent  fibres  form  only  a  part,  and  of  that 
part  the  tango-receptive  fibres  themselves  form  only  a  fraction. 
The  large  number  of  the  channels  in  the  retinal  path  is  no  doubt 
primarily  indicative  of  spatial  differentiations  of  the  receptive  sur- 
face, but  that  spatial  differentiation  is  itself  indicative  of  the 
numbers  of  the  stimuli  frequenting  that  receptive  field. 

Locomotion  and  "receptive  range"  Locomotive  progression 
and  distance  receptivity  are  two  phenomena  so  fundamentally 
correlated  that  the  physiology  of  neither  can  be  comprehended 
without  recognition  of  the  correlation  of  the  two.  Evidence 
is  forthcoming  from  ontogeny  and  phylogeny.  The  elaborate- 
ness of  the  photo-receptive  organs  of  the  flying  Insecta  corre- 
sponds with  the  great  power  of  these  forms  to  traverse  space. 
When  the  Brachiopod  passes  from  a  motile  wandering  life  to  a 
fixed  sedentary  one  its  "  eyes  "  degenerate  and  go.  The  free- 
swimming  Ascidia  with  fin-like  motor  organs  and  semi-rigid  axial 
notochord,  affording  elasticity  and  leverage,  bears  at  its  anterior 
end  a  well-formed  photo-receptor  organ  (eye)  and  a  well-formed 
otocyst  (head  proprio-ceptor).  Connected  with  the  nerves  of 
these,  the  anterior  end  of  its  truly  vertebrate  central  nervous 
system  has  a  relatively  large  *'  brain."     Thence  extends  back- 


I 


]       RECEPTIVE   RANGE  AND    LOCOMOTION       335 

ward  arbng  the  body  a  spinal  cord.  Suddenly  its  free-swimming 
habit  is  exchanged  for  a  sedentary ;  by  adhesive  projections  from 
its  head,  it  attaches  itself  permanently  to  some  fixed  object.  At 
once  there  ensues  a  re-adaptive  metamorphosis.  Degeneration 
sets  in  concurrently  in  its  locomotive  musculature,  its  eye,  its 
otocyst,  its  brain,  and  its  cord.  These  vanish  as  by  magic  save 
that  a  fraction  of  the  brain  remains  as  a  small  ganglion  near  the 
mouth.  The  sessile  creature  retains,  so  far  as  can  be  judged 
from  their  microscopic  structure,  only  some  gustatory  (?)  recep- 
tors round  the  mouth,  and  some  tango-receptors  (?  noci-ceptors) 
in  the  tegument,  connected  doubtless  with  an  irregular  diffuse 
subtegumental  layer  of  unstriped  muscle-tissue.  Experimental 
observations  seem  wanting  on  the  point,  but  we  may  presume 
that  in  this  metamorphosis  the  receptive  range  of  Ascidia  dwindles 
from  dimensions  measurable  by  all  the  distance  through  which 
its  free  motile  individual  floats  and  swims,  to  a  mere  film  of  the 
external  world,  say  a  millimeter  deep,  at  its  own  surface,  espe- 
cially round  its  mouth,  and  unextended  by  succession  of  time, 
save  passively  by  the  mere  flowing  of  the  water.  Such  instances 
illustrate  the  fundamental  connection  between  the  function  of  the 
skeletal  musculature  and  that  of  the  **  distance-receptors."  Did 
we  know  better  the  sensual  aspects  of  these  cases  the  more  sig- 
nificant doubtless  would  be  the  comparison. 

The  "  head "  as  physiologically  conceived.  As  regards  the 
objects  acting  on  the  organism  at  any  moment  through  its 
receptors,  the  extension  of  environmental  space  —  the  animal's 
receptive  range  —  is  not  equal  in  all  directions  as  measured  from 
the  organism  itself.  The  extension  is  greater  in  the  direction 
about  the  "leading"  pole..  Thus,  the  reactions  initiated  at  the 
eye  forerun  reactions  <^cf.  Loeb's  Ketten-reflexe)  that  will  in  due 
time  come  to  pass  through  other  receptor-organs.  The  visual 
receptors  are  usually  near  the  leading  pole,  and  so  placed  that 
they  see  into  the  field  whither  progression  goes.  And  simi- 
larly with  the  olfactory  receptors.  The  motor  train  behind,  the 
elongated  motor  machinery  of  the  rest  of  the  body,  is  there- 
fore from  this  point  of  view  a  motor  appendage  at  the  behest  of 
the  distance-receptor  organs  in  front.     The  segments  lying  at 


336  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

the  leading  pole  of  the  animal,  armed  as  they  are  with  the 
great  **  distance "  sense-organs,  constitute  what  is  termed  the 
"  head." 

The  proprio-ceptive  system  and  the  head.  We  may  now  at- 
tempt to  enquire  whether  this  dominance  of  the  leading  segments 
which  is  traceable  in  the  receptors  of  the  extero-ceptive  field  ap- 
plies in  the  field  of  reception  which  we  termed  the  proprio-ceptive. 
We  arrived  earlier  at  the  notion  that  the  field  of  reception  which 
extends  through  the  depth  of  each  segment  is  differentiated 
from  the  surface  field  by  two  main  characters.  One  of  these 
was  that  while  many  agents  which  act  on  the  body  surface  are 
excluded  from  the  deep  field  as  stimuli,  an  agency  which  does 
act  there  is  mass,  with  all  its  mechanical  consequences,  such  as 
weight,  mechanical  inertia,  etc.,  giving  rise  to  pressures,  strains, 
etc.,  and  that  the  receptors  of  this  deep  field  are  adapted  for 
these  as  stimuli.  The  other  character  of  the  stimulations  in  this 
field  we  held  to  be  that  the  stimuli  are  given  in  much  greater 
measure  than  in  the  surface  field  of  reception,  by  actions  of  the 
organism  itself,  especially  by  mass  movements  of  its  parts. 
Since  these  movements  are  themselves  for  the  most  part  reac- 
tions to  stimuli  received  by  the  animal's  free  surface  from  the 
environment,  the  proprio-ceptive  reactions  themselves  are  results 
in  large  degree  habitually  secondary  to  surface  stimuli.  The 
immediate  stimulus  for  the  reflex  started  at  the  deep  receptor 
is  thus  supplied  by  some  part  of  the  organism  itself  as  agent. 

In  many  forms  of  animals,  e.g.  in  Vertebrates,  there  lies  in 
one  of  the  leading  segments  a  receptor-organ  (the  labyrinth) 
derived  from  the  extero-ceptive  field,  but  later  recessed  off  from 
it;  and  this  is  combined  in  action  with  receptors  of  the  proprio- 
ceptive field  of  the  remaining  segments.  This  receptive  organ, 
like  those  of  the  proprio-ceptive  field,  is  adapted  to  mechanical 
stimuli.  It  consists  of  two  parts,  both  endowed  with  low  recep- 
tive threshold  and  with  refined  selective  differentiation.  One 
part,  the  otolith  organ,  is  adapted  to  react  to  changes  in  the 
incidence  and  degree  of  pressure  exerted  on  its  nerve-endings 
by  a  little  weight  of  higher  specific  gravity  than  the  fluid  other- 
wise filling  the  organ.      The  other  part,  the  semicircular  canals. 


IX]  THE   PROPRIOCEPTIVE   SYSTEM  337 

reacts  to  minute  mass  movements  of  fluid  contained  within  it 
'hese  two  parts  constitute  the  labyrinth.     The  incidence  and 
legree  of  pressure  of  the  otoHths   upon   their  receptive    bed 
:hange  with  changes  in  the  position  of  the  segment  in  which 
le  labyrinth  lies,  relatively  to  the  horizon  line.     Movements  of 
le  segment  likewise  stimulate  the  labyrinthine  receptors  through 
le  inertia  of  the  labyrinthine  fluid  and  the  otoliths.     By  the 
ibyrinth  are  excited  reflexes  which  adjust  the  segment  (and 
ith  it  the  head  is  usually  immovably  conjoined)  to  the  horizon 
Ine.     And   other  parts   are    similarly  reflexly  adjusted    by  it. 
'hus,  the    refined   photo-receptive  patches  in  the  head  —  the 
fetinae  —  which   conduct  reflexes  delicately  differential   in   re- 
'gard  to  space,  appropriate  for  stimuli  higher  or  lower  or  to  right 
or  to  left  in  the  photo-receptive  patch,  depend  in  their  conduct 
of  these  upon  a  more  or  less  constant  standardization  of  their 
own  normals  of  direction  in  regard  to  the  horizon  line.     These 
photo-receptive  patches  are  set  movably  in  the  head;  by  the 
action  of  muscles  they  can  retain  their  bearing  to   the  hori- 
zon, although  the  head  itself  shifts  its  relation  to  the  horizon. 
|The  control  of  these  muscles  lies  largely  with  the  labyrinth.    The 
ibyrinth  produces  a  compensatory  eyeball  reflex.     Thus  in  the 
lead  segments  the  labyrinth  effects  reflex  movements  analogous 
that  which  the  proprio-receptive  nerves  from  the  extensor 
luscles  of  the  knee  excites  in  the  leg  segments,  reflexes  restor- 
ing an  habitual  posture  that  has  been  departed  from. 

And  from  the  above  it  seems  clear  that  there  is  another  fea- 
ire  of  resemblance  between  the  labyrinthine  receptor  and  the 
>roprio-ceptors  of  the  limb.  Stimulation  of  the  labyrinth  must 
In  preponderant  measure  be  given  not  by  external  agents  directly 
but  by  the  reaction  of  the  organism  itself.  Posture  and  move- 
ment of  the  head  are  the  immediate  causes  which  stimulate  the 
labyrinth,  whether  or  not  they  be  part  of  a  total  movement  or 
posture  of  the  whole  individual.  Such  movement  is  most  fre- 
quently an  active  one  on  the  part  of  the  animal  itself.  Thus, 
when  Ascidia  becomes  sedentary  and  its  locomotor  muscalature 
atrophies  its  otocyst  disappears.  But  an  animal's  active  move- 
ment is  in  its  turn  usually  traceable  as  a  reaction  to  an  environ- 


338  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

mental  stimulus  affecting  the  receptors  at  the  surface  of  the 
animal.  Thus  the  labyrinthine  receptors  like  the  proprio-ceptors 
in  other  segments,  are  stimulated  by  the  animal  itself  as  agent, 
though  secondarily  to  stimulation  of  the  animal  itself  via  some 
extero-ceptor. 

And  there  is  another  point  of  likeness  between  labyrinth  re- 
flexes and  those  of  the  proprio-ceptors  of  the  limb  and  other 
segments.  The  proprio-ceptors  of  the  limbs  appear  productive 
of  certain  continuous,  that  is  tonic,  reflexes.  Thus,  in  the  decere- 
brate dog  the  tonic  extensor  rigidity  of  the  leg  appears  reflexly 
maintained  by  afferent  neurones  reaching  the  cord  from  the 
deep  structures  of  the  leg  itself.  Similarly,  if  the  knee-jerk  be 
accepted  as  evidence  in  the  spinal  animal  of  a  spinal  tonus  in 
the  extensor  muscle,  this  tonus  seems  maintained  by  afferent 
fibres  from  the  extensor  muscle  itself,  since  the  knee-jerk  is 
extinguished  by  severence  of  those  fibres.  Again,  the  rapidity 
of  onset  of  rigor  mortis  in  a  muscle  is  speedier  when  its  tonus 
prior  to  death  has  been  high.  Section  of  the  afferent  roots  ot 
the  limb  prior  to  death  delays  onset  of  rigor  mortis  ^^  in  that 
limb  as  judged  by  stiffness  at  the  knee ;  but  that  delay  is  not 
observable  when  skin-nerves  only  have  been  severed.  The 
labyrinthine  receptors  appear  likewise  to  be  the  source  of  cer- 
tain maintained,  that  is  tonic,  reflexes.  Destruction  of  the  laby- 
rinth also  delays  the  onset  of  rigor-mortis  in  the  muscles  to 
which  its  field  of  tonus  can  be  traced.  Ewald  has  shown  that 
each  labyrinth  maintains  tonus  especially  in  the  neck  and  trunk 
muscles  and  in  the  extensor-abductor  limb-muscles  of  the 
homonymous  side. 

In  regard  to  these  tonic  reflexes  it  is  difficult  to  see  how  a 
steady  mechanical  stimulus  can  continue  to  elicit  a  reflex  con- 
stantly for  long  periods.  If  we  take  sensation  as  a  guide,  a  touch 
excited  by  constant  mechanical  pressure  of  slight  intensity  fades 
quickly  below  the  threshold  of  sensation.  It  is  said  that  a  spinal 
frog  may  even  be  crushed  by  mechanical  pressure  without  ex- 
citing from  it  a  reflex  movement  provided  that  the  pressure 
be  applied  by  very  slowly  progressive  increments.  The  office 
of  a  receptor  would   seem  to  be,  placed  across  the  line  of  a 


:]  TONIC   REFLEXES  339 


I 

^■tream  of  energy,  to  react  under  the  transference  of  energy 
Btross  it,  as  for  instance  from  the  environment  to  the  organism, 
or  vice  versa.  We  have  many  instances  in  which  the  Hving 
material  adapts  itself  to,  and  maintains  its  own  equilibrium 
under,  different  grades  of  environmental  stress,  treating  each 
fairly  continuous  or  slowly  altering  grade  as  a  normal   zero. 

(he  slow  changes  of  barometric  pressure  on  the  body  surface 
riginate  no  skin-sensation,  though  they  are  much  above  the 
threshold  value  for  touch.  There  streams  constantly  from  the 
body  through  the  skin  a  current  of  thermal  energy  much  above 
the  threshold  value  of  stimuli  for  warmth  sensations ;  yet  this 
current  evokes  under  ordinary  circumstances  no  sensation.  It 
is  the  stationary  condition,  the  fact  that  the  transference  of 
energy  continues  at  constant  speed,  which  makes  it  unperceived. 
The  receptor  apparatus  is  not  stimulated  unless  there  is  a  change 
of  rate  in  the  transference,  and  that  change  of  rate  must  occur 
in  most  cases  with  considerable  quickness,  otherwise  there  is  a 
mere  unperceived  shift  in  the  stationary  equilibrium  which  forms 
the  resting  zero  of  the  sensual  apparatus.  Over  and  over  in  the 
elicitation  of  reflexes  as  well  as  in  the  artificial  excitation  of 
nerve  or  muscle  we  meet  this  same  feature.  Both  for  sensa- 
tion and  for  reflex  action  a  function  in  the  threshold  value  of 
stimulus  is  time  as  well  as  intensity  and  quantity.  If  a  weak 
agent  is  to  stimulate,  its  application  must  be  abrupt.  But  in 
the  tonic  reflexes  whose  source  lies  at  the  proprio-ceptors  and 
the  labyrinth  a  weak  stimulus,  although  apparently  unchanging, 
seems  to  continue  to  be  an  effective  stimulus. 

The  proprio-ceptors  and  the  labyrinthine  receptors  seem  to 
have  in  common  this,  that  they  both  originate  and  maintain  tonic 
reflexes  in  the  skeletal  muscles.  And  they,  at  least  in  some 
instances,  reinforce  one  another  in  this  action.  Thus  the  tonus 
of  the  extensor  muscle  of  the  knee  in  the  cat  and  dog  appears 
to  have  a  combined  source  in  the  proprio-ceptors  of  that  muscle 
itself  and  in  the  receptors  of  the  homonymous  labyrinth.  The 
tonus  of  skeletal  muscles  is  an  obscure  problem.  Its  mode  of 
production,  its  distribution  in  the  musculature,  its  purposive  sig- 
nificance, are  all  debateable.     The  steadiness  and  slight  inten- 


340  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

sity  of  the  contraction  constituting  the  tonus  render  its  detection 
difficult  Part  of  the  discrepancy  between  the  experimental 
findings  may  be  traced  to  the  supposition  that  a  reflex  tonus  if 
present  is  present  in  all  muscles  at  all  times.  A  single  muscle 
examined  for  reflex  tonus  has  been  taken  to  represent  all 
muscles  under  all  conditions,  although  the  answer  has  been 
sometimes  positive  and  sometimes  negative. 

It  appears  to  me  likely  that  reflex  tonus  is  the  expression  of 
a  neural  discharge  concerned  with  the  maintenance  of  attitude. 
In  many  reflex  reactions  the  eff'ect  is  movement  and  the  muscles 
are  dealt  with  as  organs  of  motion.  In  these  cases  the  stimuli 
and  the  reactions  both  of  them  are  short-lived  events.  But 
much  of  the  reflex  reaction  expressed  by  the  skeletal  muscu- 
late  is  postural.  The  bony  and  other  levers  of  the  body  are 
maintained  in  certain  attitudes  both  in  regard  to  the  horizon,  to 
the  vertical,  and  to  one  another.  The  frog  as  it  rests  squatting 
in  its  tank  has  an  attitude  far  different  from  that  which  gravita- 
tion would  give  it  were  its  musculature  not  in  action.  Evi- 
dently the  greater  part  of  the  skeletal  musculature  is  all  the  time 
steadily  active,  antagonizing  gravity  in  maintaining  the  head 
raised,  the  trunk  semi-erect,  and  the  hind  legs  tautly  flexed. 
Innervation  and  co-ordination  are  as  fully  demanded  for  the 
maintenance  of  a  posture  as  for  the  execution  of  a  movement 
This  steady  co-ordinate  innervation  antagonizes  gravitation  and 
other  forces,  e,  g.  as  in  currents  of  water.  In  these  tonic  as  in 
other  reflexes  antagonistic  muscles  co-operate  co-ordinately. 
There  is  nothing  to  show  that  reciprocal  innervation  does  not 
obtain  in  the  one  class  of  reflex  as  in  the  other.  If  so,  it  be- 
comes easily  intelligible  that  the  slight  reflex  contraction  termed 
skeletal  tonus  should  under  given  conditions  be  found  in  some 
muscles  and  not  in  others.  The  slight  reflex  contraction  will  be 
accompanied  by  reflex  inhibition  of  the  antagonistic  muscles. 
For  reflex  tonus  to  be  the  expression  of  a  neural  discharge 
which  maintains  attitude  accords  well  with  the  ascription  of  its 
source  to  the  proprio-ceptors,  including  the  labyrinth.  Those 
are  exactly  the  receptors  which,  functioning  as  sense-organs, 
initiate  sensations  of  posture  and  of  attitude  (Bonnier).     And  it 


IX]  SKELETAL   MUSCULAR  TONUS  341 

accords  also  with  the  share  in  the  production  and  regulation  of 
skeletal  tonus  which  the  cerebellum  has  (Luciani's  atonia)  and 
,the  cerebrum. 

Naturally,  the  distinction  between  reflexes  of  attitude  and 
reflexes  of  movement  is  not  in  all  cases  sharp  and  abrupt.  Be- 
/een  a  short  lasting  attitude  and  a  slowly  progressing  move- 
lent  the  difference  is  hardly  more  than  one  of  degree.  Moreover, 
jach  posture  is  introduced  by  a  movement  of  assumption,  and 
ifter  each  departure  from  the  posture,  if  it  is  resumed,  it  is  re- 
verted to  by  a  movement  of  compensation.  Hence  the  taxis  of 
ittitude  must  involve  not  only  static  reactions  of  tonic  mainte- 
lance  of  contraction,  but  innervations  which  execute  reinforcing 
^movements  and  compensatory  movements.  In  all  this  kind  of 
(function  the  proprio-ceptors  of  the  body  generally  and  of  the 
labyrinthine  receptors  in  the  head  appear  to  co-operate  together 
md  form  functionally  one  receptive  system. 

This  system  as  a  whole  may  be  embraced  within  the  one  term 
'*  proprio-ceptive."  Our  inquiry  regarding  it  is  now,  whether 
that  part  of  it  which  is  situate  in  the  leading  segments,  namely 
Its  labyrinthine  part,  exerts  preponderance  in  the  system  as  in 
[the  extero-ceptive  system  do  the  extero-ceptors  situate  in  the 
'leading  segments.  It  must  be  remembered  of  the  extero-ceptive 
system  that  even  in  the  segments  which  are  not  the  leading 
segments  its  receptors  considered  as  sense  organs  produce 
sensations  that  have  some  projicience ;  and  that  in  animals  pro- 
vided with  outstanding  skin  appendages,  e.g.  hair,  the  tango- 
reflexes  are  to  a  slight  extent  reactions  to  objects  at  a  distance. 
This  germ  of  distance  reaction  and  projicience  of  sensation  in 
the  extero-ceptors  of  the  ordinary  body-segments  is  developed 
in  the  extero-ceptors  of  the  leading  segments  into  the  vast 
distance-reactions  of  the  eye  and  the  absolute  projicience  of 
vision.  But  the  proprio-ceptors  of  the  limb  and  body  segments 
exhibit  no  germ  of  distance-reaction  nor  of  projicience  of  sensa- 
tion. And  the  specialized  proprio-ceptor  organ  of  the  leading 
segment  (the  labyrinth)  is  similarly  not  a  distance-receptor; 
although  some  of  its  sensations  seem  projected  into  the  environ- 
ment as  well  as  referred  to  the  organism  itself,  to  the  "  material 


342  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

me."  Any  predominance  this  proprio-ceptor  in  the  leading 
segments  may  exhibit  in  the  proprio-ceptive  system  is  not  there- 
fore in  virtue  of  the  quality  of  reaction  at  a  distance.  If  pre-^? 
eminently  important  to  the  organism  as  a  whole  its  pre-eminence^^ 
of  importance  rests  on  other  grounds  than  does  the  importance 
of  the  great  distance-receptors,  —  the  olfactory,  the  visual,  and 
the  auditory. 

A  posture  of  the  animal  as  a  whole  —  a  total  posture  —  is  as 
much  a  complex  built  up  of  postures  of  portions  of  the  animal 
—  segmental  postures  (Bonnier)  —  as  is  the  total  movement  of 
the  animal  —  its  locomotion  —  compounded  of  segmental  move- 
ments. With  the  hinder  part  of  its  spinal  cord  alone  intact  the 
frog  maintains  a  posture  in  its  hind  limbs.  These  limbs  are  kept 
flexed  at  hip,  knee,  and  ankle.  When  displaced  from  that  pos- 
ture they  return  to  it.  But  if  the  animal  be  rolled  over  on  its 
back  it  makes  no  attempt  to  right  itself  The  decerebrate  frog 
with  its  labyrinths  intact  and  their  arcs  still  in  connection  with 
the  skeletal  musculature  maintains  the  well-known  attitude  before 
mentioned.  If  inverted  it  at  once  reverts  to  that.  The  laby- 
rinth keeps  the  world  right-side  up  for  the  organism  by  keeping 
the  organism  right-side  up  to  its  external  world.  The  cranial 
receptors  control  the  animal's  total  posture  as  do  receptors  of 
the  hinder  musculature  the  segmental  posture  of  the  hind  limbs 
when  but  the  hind  end  of  the  spinal  cord  remains. 

Thus  the  labyrinthine  proprio-ceptors  are  largely  the  equi- 
librators  of  the  head,  and  since  the  retinal  patches  are  movably 
attached  (in  mobile  eyeballs)  to  the  head,  and  since  each  retina 
has  its  normals  of  direction  conforming  with  those  of  the  head, 
these  equilibrators  of  the  head  are  closely  connected  by  nervous 
arcs  with  the  musculature  maintaining  the  postures  of  the  eye- 
balls. The  posture  of  the  head  in  many  animals  is  dependent 
on  the  musculature  not  of  the  head  segments  themselves  but  of 
a  long  series  of  segments  behind  the  head.  In  many  forms  the 
motor  organs  that  steadily  maintain  or  passingly  modify  the 
position  of  the  head  in  regard  to  the  external  world  —  con- 
veniently indexed  by  the  line  of  direction  of  gravitation  —  are 
contributed  by  the  skeletal  musculature  of  many  post-cranial 


1 


]         TOTAL  AND    SEGMENTAL  ATTITUDE         343 


gments.  Hence  the  labyrinthine  receptor  is  in  touch  with 
all  the  segments  of  the  body,  and  these  in  a  measure  may  be 
regarded  as  appended  to  the  otic  segment.  Destruction  of  the 
labyrinth  in  the  fish,  the  frog,  the  pigeon,  the  dog  produces  not 
only  malposture  of  the  eyeball  and  the  head,  but  of  the  limbs 
and  body  as  a  whole.  The  "  knock-out  blow,"  where  the  lower 
jaw  conveys  concussion  to  the  otocyst,  reduces  in  a  moment 
a  vigorous  athlete  to  an  unstrung  bulk  of  flesh  whose  weight 

»one  determines  its  attitude,  if  indeed  a  reactionless  mass  can 
;  described  as  possessing  attitude  at  all. 
The  labyrinthine  receptors  and  their  arcs  give  the  animal  its 
definite  attitude  to  the  external  world.  The  muscular  receptors 
give  to  the  segment  —  e,  g.  hind  limb  —  a  definite  attitude  less 
in  reference  to  the  external  world  than  in  reference  to  other 
segments,  e.g.  the  rest  of  the  animal.  Our  own  sensations 
from  the  labyrinth  refer  to  some  extent,  as  said  above,  to  this 
environment,  that  is,  have  some  projected  quality ;  our  muscular 
sensations  refer  to  the  body  itself,  e.  g.  contribute  to  perceptions 
of  the  relative  flexions  or  extensions  of  our  limbs.  The  arcs  of 
the  proprio-receptor  of  the  leading  segments  control  vast  fields 
of  the  skeletal  musculature,  and  deal  with  it  as  a  whole,  while 
^■|ie  arcs  of  the  proprio-ceptors  of  the  other  segments  work  with 
^%nly  limited  regions  of  the  musculature.  Hence,  in  conformity 
with  this  the  proprio-ceptor  of  the  leading  segments  possesses 
long  internuncial  paths,  for  instance,  bulbo-spinal  from  Deiter's 
nucleus  proceeding  to  all  levels  of  the  spinal  cord. 

We  traced  the  reactions  of  proprio-ceptors  of  the  limb  to 
bear  habitually  a  secondary  relation  to  the  reactions  of  the 
extero-ceptors  of  the  limb.  Similar  secondary  relation  is  evi- 
dent also  between  the  reactions  of  the  proprio-ceptor  of  the 
leading  segments  (the  labyrinth)  and  the  reactions  of  the 
extero-ceptors  of  those  segments.  These  latter  extero-ceptors 
were  seen  to  be  distance-receptors,  and  the  reactions  of  distance 
receptors  were  seen  to  be  signalized  by  their  anticipatory  char- 
acter. From  secondary  association  with  these  distance-receptors 
the  reactions  of  the  labyrinth  come  in  their  turn  to  have  antici- 
patory  character.      They   retain,   however,  their   own   special 


344  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

features  of  equilibration  and  tonus.  The  locomotion  of  an 
animal  impelled  by  its  eye  toward  its  prey  involves  co-operation 
of  the  labyrinth  with  the  retina.  And  the  tonic  labyrinthine 
reflex  which  maintains  an  attitude  may  be  just  as  truly  an  an- 
ticipatory reaction  as  any  movement  is.  The  steady  flexed  pos- 
ture of  the  frog  directed  toward  a  fly  seen  on  the  aquarium 
wall  is  a  co-ordinate  innervation  securing  preparedness  for  the 
seizure  of  the  food.  Its  character  is  as  truly  anticipatory  as  is 
that  of  any  movement.  We  might  speak  of  the  animal  as  "  at 
rest,"  but  it  is  the  tense  quietude  of  the  hunter  watching  quarry 
rather  than  rest,  such  as  supervenes  in  sleep  and  other  conditions 
where  active  innervation  is  actually  relaxed  or  reflex  action  is 
truly  in  abeyance. 

Nervous  integration  of  a  segmental  series.  By  longitudinal 
integration  short  series  of  adjoining  segments  become  in  respect 
to  some  one  character  combined  together,  so  as  to  form  in 
respect  to  that  character  practically  a  single  organ.  It  is  con- 
venient to  speak  of  such  reflex  reactions,  confined  from  start  to 
finish  to  a  single  integrated  set  of  segments,  as  "  short  reflexes  " 
giving  "  local  reactions."  Thus  the  vertebrate  appendages  called 
limbs  are  plurisegmental,  but  the  individual  segments  constitut- 
ing the  limb  form  in  respect  of  the  limb  a  functional  group  of 
such  solidarity  that  their  reactions  in  the  limb  are  at  any  one 
time  unitary. 

The  reflexes  that  extend  beyond  the  limit  of  such  a  group  are 
on  the  other  hand  conveniently  termed  **  long  reflexes!'  And  it 
is  in  the  integration  of  long  series,  or  of  the  whole  series,  of  seg- 
ments one  with  another,  that,  apart  from  psychical  phenomena, 
the  nervous  system  seems  to  reach  its  acme  of  achievement.  Here 
it  is  that  we  see  eminently  what  Herbert  Spencer  has  insisted 
on,  namely,  that  integration  keeps  pace  with  difl'erentiation. 

In  the  segmental  series  the  nervous  concatenation  of  the 
segments  repeats  broadly  the  kind  of  association  evidenced 
within  each  segment  taken  singly.  Broadly  taken,  each  seg- 
ment has  on  the  one  hand  a  piece  of  the  extero-ceptive  field, 
a  piece  of  the  proprio-ceptive  field,  and  a  piece  of  the  intero- 
ceptive field,  though  this  last  is  wanting  in  not  a  few  segments. 


IXJ  NERVOUS   INTEGRATION  345 

On  the  other,  it  has  fractions  of  the  skeletal,  of  the  vascular, 
and  of  the  visceral  effector  organs.  Each  segment  has  muscu- 
lature and  glands  on  its  outer  and  visceral  surfaces.  Some 
segments  have  also  secretors  discharging  into  body  spaces. 
Each  of  these  sets  of  features  of  the  segments  has  in  the  series 
of  segments  a  nervous  system  of  some  functional  homogeneity. 
With  these  plurisegmental  systems  as  with  their  unisegmental 
pieces  in  the  single  segment  the  same  harmonies  of  interconnec- 
tion are  observable.  Thus,  the  nervous  arcs  embouching  into 
the  skeletal  musculature  start  chiefly  in  the  extero-ceptive  field 
in  so  far  as  concerns  execution  of  passing  movements,  in  the 
proprio-ceptive  field  in  so  far  as  concerns  tonic  postures;  and 
so  on,  as  sketched  above.  If  the  receptors  of  the  extero-ceptive 
field  are  regarded  from  the  point  of  view  of  the  nature  of  the 
agency  adequate  for  each  of  their  species,  representatives  of  each 
species  are  found  in  almost  every  segment.  In  this  way  the 
functional  properties  of  the  extero-ceptive  field  form  not  one  but 
several  multisegmental  organs  or  systems  of  organs.  In  each 
segment  exist  receptors  responsive  to  mechanical,  chemical,  and 
radiant  agencies  respectively.  There  is  thus  formed  a  tango- 
ceptive  system  to  which  practically  every  segment  contributes,  a 
thermo-ceptive  system,  a  noci-ceptive  system  ;  so  also  a  musculo- 
ceptive  system,  and  probably  the  receptors  of  the  intero-ceptive 
surface  similarly  constitute  a  homogeneous  system,  prominent 
among  their  adequate  agencies  being  those  of  chemical  quality. 
These  systems  of  receptive  arcs  present,  though  more  or  less 
compound,  a  solidarity  of  action  in  each  system  that  gives  each 
some  rank  as  a  physiological  entity. 

Restriction  of  segmental  distribution  a  factor  in  integration. 
The  impulse  to  nervous  integration  given  by  regional  restriction 
of  a  peculiar  species  of  organ  to  a  single  segment  has  especial 
force  where  that  organ  is  of  especial  importance.  This  is  the 
case  with  effector  organs  subserving  important  actions  of  con- 
summatory  (v.  s.  p.  326,  329)  type,  e.^.  a  sexual  appendage,  or 
the  mouth.  Such  organs  as  these  are  of  restricted  regional  dis- 
tribution and  subserve  important  reactions  of  consummatory  type. 
With  the  mouth  is  associated  differentiation  of  organs  around 


346  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE  BRAIN      [Lect. 

it.  Many  postures  and  movements  of  the  organism  are  advanta- 
geous or  disadvantageous  to  the  animal's  existence  mainly  inas- 
much as  they  improve  or  disimprove  the  position  or  attitude  of 
the  mouth  in  regard  to  objects  in  the  external  world.  Much  of 
the  long  series  of  movements  and  other  reactions  initiated  and 
guided  by  **  distance-receptors "  themselves  is  by-play  on  the 
way  to  a  consummatory  reaction  which  requires  an  appropriate 
placing  and  attitude  of  the  mouth.  That  there  is  only  one  mouth 
and  that  of  limited  segmental  extent  involves  co-ordination  of  the 
activities  of  many  other  segments  with  the  oral.  Integration  of 
pluri-segmental  activity  is  effected  here,  as  in  the  other  cases, 
mainly  by  the  synaptic  nervous  system.  The  fact  that  the 
mouth  is  usually  placed  near  the  leading  segments  of  the  ante- 
rior pole  is  therefore  a  further  factor  in  differentiating  the  seg- 
ments at  that  end  from  the  after-coming  train.  Thus  it  comes 
about  that  in  many  cases  the  animal  consists  of  two  portions 
broadly  different  in  character  but  complemental  the  one  to  the 
other,  the  head  and  the  trunk. 

It  is  noteworthy  that  the  increase  of  susceptibility  instanced 
by  the  distance  receptors  is  in  each  case  restricted  to  a  special 
patch,  quite  limited  in  area.  Given  a  synaptic  nervous  system, 
no  single  item  of  functional  arrangement  more  enforces  integra- 
tion of  an  individual  from  its  segments  than  the  restriction  of  a 
special  kind  of  receptor  to  a  single  area  or  segment  in  the  whole 
series.  The  motor  apparatus  of  many  segments  has  then  to 
subserve  a  single  segment,  since  that  segment  is  provided  with  a 
receptor  of  a  species  not  otherwise  possessed  by  the  individual 
at  all.  For  integrative  co-ordination  of  that  kind  the  synaptic 
nervous  system  affords  in  the  animal  economy  the  only  instru- 
ment. Only  by  the  formation  of  common  paths  can  due  advan- 
tage be  reaped  from  a  specially  refined  recipient  path  (private 
path)  of  locally  restricted  situation. 

Further,  the  condensed  setting  of  a  group  of  specialized 
receptors  favors  thpir  simultaneous  stimulation  in  groups  to- 
gether. Stimuli  even  of  small  area  then  cover  a  number  of 
receptive  points  in  the  receptive  sheet.  Thus,  ocular  images  of 
various  two-dimensional  shape  tend  to  be  better  differentiated 


:]       DIFFERENTIATION   AND   INTEGRATION       347 

the  photo-receptors  the  more  closely  the  individual  photo- 
jceptors  lie  together.  More  data  are  thus  gained  as  a  basis  for 
lifferential  reaction. 

Further,  the  juxtaposition  of  groups  of  specially  refined  recep- 
►rs  in  one  set  of  segments,  the  leading  or  head  segments,  con- 
luces  toward  their  simultaneous  stimulation  by  several  agencies 
lanating  from  one  and  the  same  environmental  object.  Thus, 
le  property  of  brightness  and  the  property  of  odor  belonging 
an  object  of  prey  may  then  better  excite  in  unison  a  reaction 
the  distant  reagent,  or  excite  more  potently  than  would  either 
property  alone.  And  movements  of  the  reagent  itself  are  then 
more  apt  to  intensify  simultaneously  the  reactions  of  its  two 
kinds  of  receptors.  The  collocation  of  the  disparate  receptors 
in  one  region  will  favor  that  which  psychologists  in  describing 
sensations  term  *'  complication,"  a  process  which  in  reflex  action 
has  a  counterpart  in  the  conjunction  of  reflexes  excited  by  recep- 
tors of  separate  species  but  of  allied  reaction.  This  alliance  of 
reaction  we  have  seen  finds  expression  as  mutual  reinforcement 
in  action  upon  a  final  common  path.  Thus  a  reaction  is  synthe- 
sized which  deals  with  the  environmental  object  not  merely  as  a 
stimulus  possessing  one  property  but  as  a  "  thing "  built  up  of 
properties.  A  reflex  is  attained  which  has  its  psychological 
analogue  in  a  sense  percept. 

The  cerebellum  is  the  head  ganglion  of  the  proprio-ceptive 
system.  If  the  basis  taken  for  classification  of  receptors  be  a 
physiological  one  with,  as  its  criterion,  the  type  of  reaction  which 
the  receptors  induce,  separate  receptive  systems  may  be  traced 
running  throughout  the  whole  series  of  segments  composing 
the  total  organism.  We  have  seen  that  such  separate  receptive 
systems  may  be  treated  as  functional  unities,  extending  through 
the  segmental  series.  In  any  such  system  there  is  evident  a  ten- 
dency for  its  central  nervous  mechanisms,  that  is  to  say,  the  com- 
ponents of  the  central  nervous  organ  which  specially  accrue  to 
the  system  in  question,  to  be  gathered  chiefly  where  the  most 
important  contribution  to  its  receptive  paths  enters  the  central 
nervous  system.  The  receptive  system  in  question  has  as  it 
were  its  focus  at  that  place.     Thus  receptive  neurones  which 


348  THE   DOMINANCE   OF  THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

can  influence  respiratory  movement  enter  the  central  nervous 
organ  at  various  segments,  but  the  chief  respiratory  centre  lies 
in  the  bulb  where  the  receptive  neurones  from  the  lung  itself 
make  entrance  and  central  connection,  the  vagal  receptors  being 
preponderantly  regulative  in  that  function.  And  we  have  seen 
that  a  proprio-ceptive  organ  (the  labyrinth)  in  the  head  seg- 
ments seems  preponderantly  regulative  in  those  functions  which 
the  proprio-ceptive  system  subserves.  The  central  neural  mech- 
anism belonging  to  the  proprio-ceptive  system  is  preponder- 
antly built  up  over  the  central  connections  of  this  proprio-ceptive 
organ  (the  labyrinth)  belonging  to  the  head.  Thither  converge 
internuncial  paths  stretching  to  this  mechanism  from  the  central 
endings  of  various  proprio-ceptive  neurones  situate  in  all  the 
segments  of  the  body.  There  afferent  contributions  from  the 
receptors  of  joints,  muscles,  ligaments,  tendons,  viscera,  etc., 
combine  with  those  from  the  muscular  organs  of  the  head  and 
with  those  of  the  labyrinthine  receptors  themselves.  A  central 
nervous  organ  of  high  complexity  results.  Its  size  from  animal 
species  to  animal  species  strikingly  accords  with  the  range  and 
complexity  of  the  habitual  movements  of  the  species ;  in  other 
words,  with  the  range  and  complexity  of  the  habitual  taxis  of  the 
skeletal  musculature.     This  central  organ  is  the  cerebellum. 

The  symptoms  produced  by  its  destruction  or  injury  in  whole 
or  in  part  in  many  ways  resemble,  therefore,  the  disturbances  pro- 
duced by  injury  of  the  labyrinth  itself.  It  also  influences  tonus 
very  much  as  do  the  simple  proprio-ceptive  arcs  themselves. 
It  is  closely  connected  structurally  and  functionally  with  the  so- 
called  motor  region  of  the  cerebral  hemisphere,  just  as  the 
simpler  proprio-ceptive  arcs  and  reflexes  are  closely  associated 
with  the  mechanisms  of  extero-ceptive  reactions.  Knowledge  is 
not  ripe  as  yet  for  an  adequate  definition  of  the  function  of  the 
cerebellum.  Many  authorities  have  defined  it  as  the  centre  for 
the  maintenance  of  the  mechanical  equilibrium  of  the  body. 
Others  regard  it  as  the  organ  for  co-ordination  of  volitional 
movement.  Spencer  suggested  that  it  was  the  organ  of  co-ordi- 
nation of  bodily  action  in  regard  to  space,  the  cerebrum  he 
suggested  being  the  organ  of  co-ordination  of  bodily  action  in 


THE   CEREBRUM  349 

spect  of  time.  Lewandowski  considers  it  the  central  organ  for 
e  "  muscular  sense."  Luciani,  the  universally  acknowledged 
thority  on  the  physiology  of  the  cerebellum,  describes  it  as 
e  organ  which  by  unconscious  processes  exerts  a  continual 
enforcing  action  on  the  activity  of  all  other  nerve-centres. 

It  is  instructive  to  note  how  all  these  separate  pronounce- 
ents  harmonize  with  the  supposition  that  the  organ  is  the  chief 
co-ordinative  centre  or  rather  group  of  centres  of  the  reflex 
system  of  proprio-ception.  The  cerebellum  may  indeed  be 
described  as  the  head-ganglion  of  the  proprio-ceptive  system, 
and  the  head  ganglion  here,  as  in  other  systems,  is  the  main 

Iranglion. 
The  cerebrum  is  the  ganglion  of  the  ''  distance-receptors."  By 
he  **  distance-receptors  "  are  initiated  and  guided  long  series  of 
eactions  of  the  animal  as  a  whole.  Other  receptive  reactions 
ptegrate  individual  segments;  the  reactions  of  the  distance- 
ieceptors  integrate  the  whole  series  of  segments.  It  is  in  the 
sphere  of  reactions  of  these  "  distance-receptors  "  that  the  most 
subtle  and  complex  adjustments  of  the  animal  therefore  arise. 
In  their  neural  machinery  not  only  short  arcs  but  long  arcs,  in- 

^ Giving  extensive  internuncial  tracts,  figure  largely.  Chains  of 
eaction  conducive  to  a  final  reaction  relatively  remote  are  more 
evident  with  them  than  with  other  arcs.  If  appeal  to  psychical 
evidence  be  ventured  on  it  is  to  the  field  of  operation  of  the 
arcs  of  these  distance-receptors  that  higher  feats  of  associative 
memory  accrue,  and,  though  the  phrase  is  hardly  permissible 
here  except  w^ith  curtailed  scope,  conation  becomes  more  intel- 
ligent. Finally,  in  harmony  with  the  last  inference,  it  is  over 
these  "  distance-receptors  "  and  in  connection  with  their  reflexes 
and  arcs  that  the  cerebrum  itself  is  found.  The  cerebrum 
constitutes,  so  to  say,  the  ganglion  of  the  "  distance-receptors." 
Langendorff"^  has  pointed  out  that  a  blinded  frog  resembles  in 
its  reactions  a  frog  with  the  cerebrum  removed:  the  elasmo- 
branch  without  its  olfactory  lobes  behaves  as  if  it  had  lost  its  fore 
brain.  Edinger  traces  the  genesis  of  the  cerebral  cortex  to  a 
distance-receptor,  namely  the  olfactory  organ. 

The  integration  of  the  animal  associated  with  these  "  distance- 


k 


350  THE  DOMINANCE  OF  THE   BRAIN       [Lect. 

receptors  "  of  the  leading  segments  can  be  briefly  with  partial 
justice  expressed  by  saying  that  the  rest  of  the  animal,  so  far  as 
its  motor  machinery  goes,  is  but  the  servant  of  them.  We  might 
imagine  the  form  of  the  individual  and  the  disposition  of  the 
sense-organs  as  primitively  very  simple ;  for  instance,  a  spheroid 
with  a  digestive  cavity  and  sense-organs  distributed  especially 
over  the  external  surface.  Such  an  imaginary  form  we  should 
expect  under  evolution  to  become  modified.  If  a  motile  organ- 
ism, its  contractile  mechanisms  would  obtain  mechanical  ad- 
vantage (leverage)  by  its  elongation  in  certain  directions.  The 
lengthwise  extension  of  the  vertebrate  body  and  of  its  lateral 
motor  appendages,  e.  g.  limbs,  are  in  so  far  such  as  might  be 
argued  a  priori.  Under  evolution  in  motile  animals  adaptations 
securing  appropriate  leverage  for  the  contractile  apparatus 
appear,  and  length  along  certain  axes  is  always  a  consideration 
in  them.  In  animals  with  segments  ranged  along  a  single  axis, 
the  animal  for  the  greater  part  of  its  length  comes  to  be  one 
great  motor  organ,  complex  and  able  to  execute  movements  in 
various  ways,  but  still  a  unity.  The  pole  at  which  the  great 
"  distance-receptors "  (visual,  olfactory,  auditory)  lie  is  that 
which,  in  the  habitual  locomotion  of  the  animal  under  the  action 
of  the  motor  train  attached,  "  leads."  The  animal  therefore 
moves  habitually  into  that  part  of  environmental  space  which 
has  been  already  explored  by  the  distance-receptors  of  its  own 
leading  segments. 

The  head  is  in  many  ways  the  individual's  greater  part.  It 
is  the  more  so  the  higher  the  individual  stands  in  the  animal 
scale.  It  has  the  mouth,  it  takes  in  the  food,  including  water 
and  air,  it  has  the  main  receptive  organs  providing  data  for  the 
rapid  and  accurate  adjustment  of  the  animal  to  time  and  space. 
To  it  the  trunk,  an  elongated  motor  organ  with  a  share  of  the 
digestive  surface,  and  the  skin,  is  appended  as  an  apparatus  for 
locomotion  and  nutrition.  The  latter  must  of  necessity  lie  at 
the  command  of  the  great  receptor-organs  of  the  head.  The 
co-ordination  of  the  activities  of  the  trunk  with  the  requirements 
of  the  head  is  a  cardinal  function  of  the  synaptic  nervous  system. 
Conducting  arcs  must  pass  from  the  cephalic  receptors  to  the 


IX]  THE   NERVOUS   SUPERSTRUCTURE  351 

contractile  masses  of  the  body  as  a  whole.  The  spinal  cord 
contains  these  strands  of  conductors  in  vertebrates  and  is  from 
this  point  of  view  a  mere  appendage  of  the  brain.  A  salient 
feature  of  these  conducting  arcs  is  that  the  nerve-fibres  from  the 
cephalic  receptors  do  not  run,  as  might  perhaps  a  priori  have 
J^en  thought  natural,  direct  from  their  cephalic  segment  back- 
^■ards  to  reach  the  common  effector  paths  upon  which  they 
^■nbouch.  Instead  of  having  that  arrangement,  these  fibres, 
^ftarting  in  the  cephalic  receptors,  end  in  the  gray  matter  of  the 
Central  nervous  axis  not  far  from  their  own  segment.  Thence 
the  conducting  arc  is  continued  backward  by  another  strand  of 
fibres,  and  these  reach  (perhaps  directly)  the  mouths  of  the 
final  common  paths  in  the  gray  matter  of  segments  of  the  spinal 
cord.  This  is  the  arrangement  exemplified  by  the  pulmono- 
phrenic  and  other  respiratory  arcs,  the  depresso-splanchnic 
arcs,  the  olfacto-phrenic  respiratory  arcs,  the  arcs  between  the 
otic  labyrinth  and  the  muscles  maintaining  posture  in  the  trunk, 
and  practically  that  of  the  retino-molor  arcs  connecting  the 
retina  with  the  muscles  of  the  neck.  It  gives  at  least  one  synap- 
sis more  than  the  first  alternative  would  do.  And  each  synapse 
is  an  apparatus  for  co-ordination ;  it  introduces  a  ^'  common  path'' 
And  it  is  in  the  exercise  of  the  distance-receptors  with  their 
extensive  range  overlapping  that  of  other  receptors  that  the 
reflexes  which  relate  to  "  objects  "  in  the  sense  that  they  are 
reflexes  synthesized  from  receptors  of  separate  species  become 
chiefly  established.  The  ramifications  of  the  central  neurones 
attached  to  these  receptors  are  so  extensive  and  the  reactions 
they  excite  are  so  far  spreading  in  the  organism  that  their  asso- 
ciation with  the  reactions  and  central  mechanisms  of  other  recep- 
tors is  especially  frequent  and  wide. 

The  distance-receptors  are  the  great  inaugurators  of  reaction. 
The  reduced  initiation  of  action  which  ensues  on  ablation  of  the 
cerebrum  seems  explicable  by  that  reason.  The  curtailment 
which  ensues  is  indicative  of  damage  which  their  removal  inflicts 
on  reactions  generated  by  the  distance-receptor  organs.  By  a 
high  spinal  transection  the  splendid  motor  machinery  of  the 
vertebrate  is  practically  as  a  whole  and  at  one  stroke  severed 


352  THE   DOMINANCE   OF   THE   BRAIN      [Lect. 

from  all  the  universe  except  its  own  microcosm  and  an  environ- 
mental film  some  millimeters  thick  immediately  next  its  body. 
The  deeper  depression  of  reaction  into  which  the  higher  animal 
as  contrasted  with  the  lower  sinks  when  made  spinal  signifies 
that  in  the  higher  types  more  than  in  the  lower  the  great  dis- 
tance-receptors actuate  the  motor  organ  and  impel  the  actions 
of  the  individual.  The  deeper  depression  shows  that  as  the 
individual  ascends  the  scale  of  being  the  more  reactive  does  it 
become  as  an  individual  to  the  circumambient  universe  outside 
itself.  It  is  significant  that  spinal  shock  hardly  at  all  affects  the 
nervous  reactions  of  the  intero-ceptors  (visceral  system) ;  and 
that  it  does  not  affect  the  intero-ceptive  arcs  appreciably  more 
in  the  monkey  than  in  the  frog.  Its  brunt  falls,  as  we  have  seen 
before,  on  the  reactions  of  the  skeletal  musculature.  Not  that 
in  the  highest  animal  forms  the  "distance-receptor"  merely  per 
se  has  necessarily  reached  more  perfection  or  more  competence 
than  in  the  lower.  In  lower  types,  as  in  fish,  are  found 
"  distance-receptors  "  of  high  perfection,  but  their  ablation  does 
not  in  lower  types  cripple  in  the  same  way  as  in  higher  types. 
It  is  that  in  the  higher  types  there  is  based  upon  the  "  distance- 
receptors  "  a  relatively  enormous  neural  superstructure  possess- 
ing million-sided  connections  with  multitudinous  other  nervous 
arcs  and  representing  untold  potentialities  for  redistribution  of 
so-to-say  stored  stimuli  by  associative  recall.  The  development 
and  elaboration  of  this  internal  nervous  mechanism  attached  to 
the  organs  of  distance-reception  has,  so  far  as  we  can  judge,  far 
outstripped  progressive  elaboration  of  the  peripheral  receptive 
organs  themselves.  Adaptation  and  improvement  would  seem 
to  have  been  more  precious  assets  in  the  former  than  in  the 
latter.  And,  as  related  to  the  former  rather  than  to  the  latter, 
must  be  regarded  the  parallelism  of  the  ocular  axes  and  the 
overlapping  of  the  uniocular  fields  of  photo-reception  which  in 
mammals  has  gradually  reached  its  acme  in  the  monkey  and 
in  man.  This  overlapping  yields,  in  virtue  one  would  think  of 
some  process  akin  to  Herbart's  "  complication,"  an  important 
additional  datum  for  visual  space.  This,  together  with  promo- 
tion of  the  fore  limb  from  a  simple  locomotor  prop  to  a  delicate 


:] 


THE   CEREBRUM 


353 


explorer  of  space  in  manifold  directions,  together  also  with  the 
organization  of  mimetic  movement  to  express  thoughts  by  sounds, 
lave  with  the  developments  of  central  nervous  function  which 
ley  connote  and  promote  been  probably  the  chief  factors  in 
lan's  outstripping  other  competitors  in  progress  toward  that 
lim  which  seems  the  universal  goal  of  animal  behavior,  namely 
dominate  more  completely  the  environment.     Remembering 
lese  conditions,  it  need  not  surprise  us  that  the  distance-recep- 
>rs  more  and  more  exert  preponderant  directive  influence  over 
le  whole  nervous  system.     To  say  this  is  to  say  no  more  than 
lat  the  motile  and  consolidated  individual  is  driven,  guided, 
id  controlled  by,  above  all  organs,  its  cerebrum.     The  inte- 
grating power  of  the  nervous  system  has  in  fact  in  the  higher 
inimal,  more  than  in  the  lower,  constructed  from  a  mere  collec- 
^on  of  organs  and  segments  a  functional  unity,  an  individual  of 
lore  perfected  solidarity.     We  see  that  the  distance-receptors 
integrate  the  individual  not  merely  because  of  the  wide  ramifi- 
lation  of  their  arcs  to  the  effector   organs  through  the  lower 
:entres ;   they  integrate  especially  because  of  their  great  con- 
lections  in  the  high  cerebral  centres.     Briefly  expressed,  their 
special  potency  is  because  they  integrate  the  animal  through  its 
)rain.     The   cerebrum    itself  may  be   indeed  regarded  as  the 
ganglion  of  the  distance-receptors. 


23 


354  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 


LECTURE    X 

SENSUAL  FUSION 

Argument :  Nervous  integration  in  relation  to  bodily  movement  and  to 
sensation  compared.  Sensual  fusion  in  a  relatively  simple  instance 
of  binocular  vision.  The  rotating  binocular  lantern.  Flicker  sen- 
sations generated  at  "  corresponding  retinal  points  " ;  absence  of 
evidence  of  their  summation  or  interference  either  with  synchronous 
or  asynchronous  flicker  of  similar  frequency.  Their  interference 
when  the  flicker  is  of  dissimilar  frequency.  Talbot's  law  not  ap- 
plicable to  "  corresponding  points."  Fechner's  paradox.  Preva- 
lence of  contours  under  Weber's  law  and  under  binocular  summation 
compared.  The  physiological  initial  stages  of  the  reaction  generated 
in  either  of  a  pair  of  corresponding  retinal  points  proceeds  without 
touching  the  apparatus  of  the  twin  point.  Only  after  the  sensations 
initiated  from  the  right  and  left  "  points  "  have  been  elaborated  so  far 
as  to  be  well  amenable  to  introspection  does  interference  between 
the  reactions  of  the  two  (right  and  left)  eye-systems  occur.  The  con- 
vergence of  nerve-paths  from  the  right  and  left  retinae  respectively 
toward  one  cerebral  region  is  significant  of  union  for  co-ordination 
of  motor  reaction  rather  than  for  synthesis  of  sensation.  Resem- 
blances between  motor  and  sensual  reactions.  The  cerebrum  pre- 
eminently the  organ  of  and  for  the  adaptation  of  reactions. 

The  animal  whose  nervous  construction  we  have  been  attempt- 
ing to  follow  thus  far,  we  have  supposed  merely  a  puppet  moved 
by  the  external  world  in  which  it  is  immersed  ;  and  we  have  sup- 
posed it  a  puppet  without  passions,  memory,  feelings,  sensations, 
let  alone  ideas  concrete  or  abstract.  From  time  to  time  we  have 
purposely  invoked  appeal  to  sensations  and  feelings  such  as  our 
own  experience  of  ourselves  provides  in  order  to  see  better 
whither  lead  the  blind  reactions  of  the  thing  that  we  have  been 
imagining  a  fatal  mechanism.  Whether  such  sensations  or  feelings 
accompany  or  do  not  accompany  the  reactions  we  have  been 
studying  we  have  left  open.  We  have  tacitly  consented  that  our 
point  of  study  of  those  reactions  leaves  that  question,  to  which 
the  present  time  gives  no  clear  answer,  as  one  with  which  we  are 
not  concerned.     But  we  may  agree  that  if  such  sensations  and 


'X]  REFLEX   UNION  355 


I 

H  feelings  or  anything  at  all  closely  like  them  do  accompany  the 
H  reactions  we  have  studied,  the  neural  machinery  to  whose  working 
H  they  are  adjunct  lies  not  confined  in  the  nervous  arcs  we  have  so 
H  far  traced  but  in  fields  of  nervous  apparatus  that,  though  connected 
^B  with  those  arcs,  lie  beyond  them,  in  the  cerebral  hemispheres. 
^B  In  the  attempt  to  trace  the  integrative  work  of  the  nervous 
»^  system  on  its  motor  side,  one  of  our  leading  principles  has  been 
^  that  of  the  "  correlation  of  reflexes  about  a  final  common  path!' 
I^p  Owing  to  the  convergence  of  many  various  reflex-arcs  toward 
and  their  confluence  in  a  common  eff*erent  path  co-ordination  in 

I  their  use  of  that  path  obtains  and  is  demonstrable. 
It  has  been  shown  that  some  reflexes  are  so  correlated  in  regard 
to  a  final  common  path  that  their  actions  on  it  coalesce  and  rein- 
force each  other.  These  are  allied  reflexes  and  have  allied  arcs. 
Good  examples  of  allied  reflexes  and  arcs  are  those  which  arise 
in  receptors  of  one  species  distributed  in  one  regional  locality  and 
subserving  one  and  the  same  type-reflex ;  such  are  the  arcs  from 
the  shoulder  region  of  the  dog  subserving  the  scratch-reflex. 
We  have  also  seen  that  reflexes  which  use  the  same  "  final 
common  path  "  but  use  it  to  difl*erent  or  opposed  effect  are  so 
correlated  in  regard  to  it  that  one  reflex  can  temporarily  inhibit 
the  other  from  use  of  the  path.  These  reflexes  we  termed  in 
regard  to  each  other  antagonistic. 
From  these  motor  reactions  it  is  natural  to  attempt  to  cross 
the  gulf  from  movement  to  sensation.  In  the  bulbo-spinal  dog 
we  may  produce  a  flexion  of  the  fore  limb  by  stigmatic  stimula- 
tion of  the  outer  digit.  A  reflex  in  its  motor  expression  to  all 
outward  appearance  like  the  preceding  we  may  also  provoke  by 
simultaneous  stimulation  of  the  skin  of  the  innermost  together 
with  that  of  the  outermost  digit.  Or  we  may  evoke  a  similar 
reflex  in  the  limb  by  stimulating  simultaneously  with  the  fore 
foot  the  opposite  hind  foot.  Here  there  is  no  conflict  between 
the  reactions  to  the  component  stimuli.  We  may  add  further 
the  simultaneous  stimulation  of  the  same  side  pinna.  The  reflex 
is  then  of  more  compound  origin,  but  its  component  reflexes  are 
so  correlated  about  their  ^^  final  common  paths''  to  the  fore  limb 
that  their  actions  there  coalesce  and  reinforce.    Further,  we  may 


356  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

add  to  these  stimuli  others  applied  to  receptors  of  species  actu- 
ally different  from  any  of  the  cutaneous  and  thus  still  further  add 
to  the  sources  of  the  total  reflex ;  and  we  may  choose  sources 
which  are  harmonious  and  the  impulses  from  them  flow  together 
and  combine. 

On  the  other  hand,  instead  of  adding  factors  that  tend  to 
combine  in  the  production  of  a  particular  reflex  we  may  excite 
simultaneously  with  other  sources  a  source  whose  reaction  is 
incompatible  with  theirs.  Then  struggle  and  rivalry  ensue  and 
the  result  may  be  inhibition  of  that  particular  reflex  movement 
and  appearance  of  some  other. 

It  appears  to  follow  from  such  considerations  that  when  we 
find  electrical  excitation  of  certain  spots  of  the  cerebral  hemi- 
sphere regularly  evoke  certain  movements,  e.g.  of  a  limb,  the 
probability  is  that  we  have  there  a  nodal  point  where  various 
harmoniously  acting  neural  arcs  are  tied  together  and  can  be 
there  reached  and  driven  as  a  unit  —  though  a  highly  syn- 
thesized one  —  and  produce  the  effect  which  is  the  common 
resultant  of  them  all. 

The  receptive  points  and  organs  which  under  stimulation 
initiate  reflex  movements  also  initiate,  in  the  intact  animal  with 
unmutilated  brain,  sensations.  As  each  reflex  has  a  reflex  action 
attributive  to  it,  so  it  has  potentially  at  least  a  sensual  reaction. 
These  sensual  reactions,  like  the  motor  reflexes,  are  of  various 
grades  of  complexity.  The  simple  perceptual  image  of  an  object 
is  usually  a  resultant  as  regards  external  stimulation  of  stimuli 
applied  jointly  to  several  sense-organs.  It  has  direct  sensual 
factors  traceable  from  various  sources.  The  cigar  taken  from  its 
box  may  be  simultaneously  sensed  through  eye  and  hand  and  nose 
and  ear.  The  object  experientially  regarded  as  a  single  object 
excites  a  neural  reaction  that  has  its  starting  points  in  many 
spatially  and  qualitatively  distinct  receptive  points,  each  point 
the  commencement  of  separate  nervous  arcs.  In  the  psychical 
result  of  the  reactions  thus  set  going  there  is  amplification  and 
modification  by  conditions  memorial,  affective,  judicial,  conative, 
etc.,  obtaining  in  the  mind  and  not  due  immediately  to  the  stim- 
ulus.    The  neural  process  resulting  from  the  nervous  impulses 


X]  BINOCULAR  FUSION  357 

litiated  by  the  retinal,  olfactory,  cutaneous,  and  muscular  recep- 
>rs  is  therefore  internally  modified  in  the  nervous  system  by 
processes  and  states  already  existent  there  or  evoked  there  by 
jelf  as  a  reverberation  of  its  action.  Can  we  at  all  compare 
rith  the  simultaneous  co-ordination  of  the  nervous  factors  in  a 
lotor  reflex  the  synthesis  of  the  nervous  elements  whose  com- 
Mnation  underlies  a  simple  sense-perception  ? 

We  may  somewhat   reduce   the   complexity  of  the  sense- 
►ercept  by  limiting  its  paths  of  initiation  to  those  of  a  single 
inse,  namely  the   visual,  excluding  the   object,  e.g,  the  seen 
igar,  from  directly  stimulating  other  sense  channels,  tactual, 
olfactory,  auditory,  muscular,  etc.     The   cigar  may  be  offered 
mly  to  the   eye.     Then  we  have   left  as  regards  the  external 
Emulation  merely  the  fusion  of  the  right-eye  and  the  left-eye 
lages.     This  fusion  is  so  complete  that  we  cannot  by  intro- 
jection  discriminate  in  the  visual  image  the  right-eye  image 
from  the  left-eye  image.     Moreover,  this  fusion  is  so  elemental 
that  introspection   cannot   detect   in    it  any  effort  of  memory, 
judgment,  or  reason.     It  appears  innate  —  a  datum  ready  pro- 
vided even  at  dawn  of  individual  human  consciousness.     We 
:an  further  strip  the  problem  of  some  of  its  complexity  by  sub- 
'stituting  for  the  three-dimensional  object,  e.  g.  the  cigar,  with 
its  perspective  shading  and  its  patches  of  colour  and  its  char- 
acteristic associations,  etc.,  a  simple  and  relatively  meaningless 
discoid  patch  of  moderate,  even,  and  uncoloured    brightness, 
small  enough  to  lie  wholly  on  the  central  area  of  each  retina. 
We  can  then  test  to  what  degree  the  visual  singleness  of  the 
observed  surface,  sensed  through  right  eye  and  left  eye  together, 
is  due  to  direct  confluence  of  the  sensory  paths  excited  by  the 
right-eye   and   left-eye  images   respectively.     We  can  attempt 
this  in  the  following  way.^^^ 

A  double  sheet  of  thick  milk-glass  is  observed  by  transmitted 
light  given  by  a  lamp.  This  lamp  is  set  in  the  axis  of  a  rotating 
cylinder  (Fig.  'j6).  In  the  side  of  the  cylinder  are  three  horizontal 
rows  of  rectangular  windows,  tier  above  tier.  The  lamp,  though 
fixed  in  the  axis  of  rotation  of  this  revolving  cylindrical  screen, 
is  entirely  free  from  all  attachment  to  it.     The  milk-glass  plate 


358 


SENSUAL  FUSION 


[Lect. 


Figure  76.  —  Rotating  Lantern.  I.  Elevation  seen  from  front.  II.  Horizontal  plan, 
through  level  of  A-A  of  I.  Supports  seen  in  perspective.  The  eyeballs,  pupil  screens, 
and  convergant  visual  axes  are  indicated  belonging  to  II,  but  carried  through  I.  The 
plan  of  the  lantern  is  given  one  fourth  actual  size.     Description  in  text. 


xj 


THE   ROTATING  BINOCULAR  LAMP 


359 


I 


is  fixed  between  the  lamp  and  the   inner  face  of  the  tiers  of 
windows,  close  to  the  latter. 

Outside  the  moving  cylindrical  screen  is  a  fixed  semi- 
cylindrical  screen  concentric  with  the  revolving  one,  and  just 
wide  enough  to  allow  the  inner  revolving  one  to  turn  within  it 
freely  (Fig.  76).  In  the  fixed  cylindrical  screen  four  circular 
holes  are  arranged  so  that  two  are  centred  on  the  same  horizon- 
tal line,  and  of  the  other  two  one  is  centred  just  so  far  above  the 
left-hand  hole  of  the  just  mentioned  pair  as  the  other  is  below 
the  right-hand  member  of  the  pair.  The  horizontal  distance 
between  the  centres  of  the  right  and  left  hand  holes  is  9  mm. 
The  diameter  of  each   hole  is  8  mm.     The    vertical   distance 


wmm/m. 


between  the  centres  of  the  holes  is  exactly  the  same  as  that  be- 
tween the  centres  of  tiers  of  the  revolving  cylindrical  screen, 
namely  1 1  mm.  These  four  circular  holes  in  the  outer  fixed 
cylindrical  screen  are,  in  the  experiments,  viewed  from  a  distance 
such  that  when  the  line  of  visual  direction  of  the  right  eye 
passes  through  the  centre  of  the  right  hole  it  meets  (Fig.  76) 
at  the  axis  of  the  cylindrical  lantern  the  line  of  visual  direction 
of  the  left  eye,  which  latter  Hne  passes  through  the  centre  of  the 
left-hand  hole. 


36o  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

This  being  so,  the  images  of  the  lower  left-hand  hole  and  of 
the  upper  right-hand  hole  fuse  visually  to  singleness.  They 
then  appear  as  the  middle  one  of  three  arranged  vertically  one 
above  the  other. 

A  black  vertical  thin  screen  set  at  right-angles  to  the  plane 
of  the  forehead  is  introduced  (Fig.  ']'])  between  the  eyes  and  the 
holes  so  as  to  screen  from  the  left  eye  all  view  of  the  right-hand 
holes,  and  from  the  right  eye  all  view  of  the  left-hand  holes. 

The  revolving  screen  is  driven  by  an  electromotor.  The 
speed  of  revolution  of  this  motor  is  controlled  by  a  coarse  ad- 
justment and  by  a  fine  adjustment.  The  speed  of  rotation  of 
the  cylindrical  screen  is  recorded  by  marking  the  completion 
of  each  revolution  of  its  spindle  by  an  electromagnetic  signal 
writing  on  a  travelling  blackened  surface  (Fig.  'j']).  On  the 
same  surface  the  time  is  recorded  by  a  writing-clock  marking 
fifths  of  seconds. 

The  inner  revolving  screen  by  its  revolution  opens  and  shuts 
alternately  for  equal  periods  the  circular  holes  in  the  fixed  outer 
screen.  The  inner  screen  with  its  three  tiers  of  windows  is  made 
in  three  pieces,  each  containing  one  tier  of  the  windows.  The 
piece  containing  the  middle  tier  of  openings  is  jointed  in  such  a 
way  that  its  openings  can  be  set  at  any  desired  interval  with 
the  openings  of  the  lowest  tier.  The  highest  tier  is  similarly 
jointed  to  the  middle  tier.  In  this  way  it  can  be  arranged  that  the 
uppermost  circular  hole  is  open  when  the  lower  ones  are  closed, 
or  is  shut  when  the  lower  are  closed,  or  is  opened  to  any  desired 
degree  either  before  or  after  the  lower;  further,  by  removing 
the  top  gallery  of  the  rotating  screen  it  can  be  left  permanently 
open.  A  similar  relationship  is  also  allowed  between  the  middle 
holes  and  the  lower. 

By  wearing  weak  prisms  with  their  base-apex  lines  vertical 
the  images  of  the  right-hand  and  left-hand  holes  can  be  brought 
to  the  same  horizontal  levels.  The  observer  can  then  immedi- 
ately fuse  the  four  images  to  two  by  convergence.  A  horizontal 
fine  thread  halving  each  of  the  two  middle  holes,  and  similar 
but  vertical  threads  halving  the  other  two  holes,  serve  to  certify 
binocular  vision  to  the  observer.     When  the  four  holes  are  all 


I 


BINOCULAR  FLICKER  361 


allowed  to  act  thus  under  appropriate  convergent  binocular 
gaze  they  are  seen  by  the  observer  as  two  evenly  lighted  discs, 
one  vertically  above  the  other,  and  each  cut  into  quadrants  by  a 
delicate  black  cross.  By  separately  adjustable  shutters  any  one, 
or  any  vertically  edged  fraction  of  one,  of  the  discs  can  be  sep- 
rately  screened  out  of  vision. 

The  object  of  the  above  arrangement  is  to  attain  the  fol- 
lowing conditions.  Images  accurately  similar  are  received  by 
retinal  areas  fully  visually  conjugate.  The  areas  are  not  only 
of  the  so-called  "  geometrical  identity,"  but  are  at  the  time  of 
the  observation  in  full  binocular  co-operation,  owing  to  the  con- 
current convergence  and  accommodation.  Extinction  and  illu- 
mination of  the  images  occur  pari  passu  in  the  two  eyes,  i.  e. 
with  like  speed  and  in  like  direction.  It  can  be  synchronous  or 
of  any  time-sequence  desired.  That  the  speed  shall  be  similar  for 
the  two  is  insured  by  all  the  shutters  being  on  the  same  spindle. 

Each  disc-shaped  image  will  have  on  the  retina  a  diameter 
of  about  570/Lt.  That  is,  when  foveal  vision  is  directed  upon  it, 
the  image  will  occupy  a  practically  rod-free  area  containing 
about  2,800  cones.  The  direction  of  translation  being  the  same 
for  all  the  shutters  the  bright  images  on  the  two  retinae  are,  if 
the  shutters  are  set  for  simultaneous  right  and  left  images,  com- 
menced on  "  identical "  points  of  the  two  retinae,  established 
progressively  along  *'  identical "  points,  and  finally  extinguished 
in  like  manner  progressively  along  **  identical  "  points.  Or,  con- 
versely, if  the  shutters  are  set  for  accurately  alternate  right  and 
left  images  the  screening  off  begins  in  one  eye  at  a  spot  and 
moment  identical  with  those  at  which  the  turning  on  of  the 
image  commences  in  the  other  eye;  so  similarly  it  finishes. 
With  the  speeds  of  revolution  used  for  the  observations  the  time 
the  shutter  takes  to  expose  or  occlude  completely  each  bright 
disc,  varies  between  .011'' and  .002'^  Error  that  might  arise 
on  this  score  is  avoided  by  the  consensual  direction  of  move- 
ment of  the  right  and  left  hand  shutters. 

That  the  "  retinal  points  "  to  which  the  images  are  thus  ap- 
plied synchronously  or  in  desired  sequence  are  truly  "  identical " 
is  certified,  (i)  by  the  paired  physical  images  being  seen  single; 


362  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

(2)  by  the  maximum  disparation  of  the  edges  of  the  rotating 
shutters  being  about  7^  on  the  retina,  whereas  350/^  is  about 
the  vertical  retinal  disparation  which  Hmits  binocular  combi- 
nation. Moreover,  a  contour  travelling  through  a  visual  angle 
of  2°  in  -^",  as  in  these  observations,  is  not  perceptible  as  a 
contour  at  all. 

Difficulties  due  to  change  in  pupil-width  are  excluded  by 
artificial  pupils.  Equality  of  brightness  of  illumination  of  the 
four  milk-glass-backed  8mm.  holes  is  obtained  by  making  the 
straight-wire  candle-shaped  lamp  of  considerable,  /.  e.  12  cm. 
length,  and  fixing  it  accurately  in  the  axis  of  the  cylindrical 
screen.  The  rotating  screen  is  blackened  inside  to  minimize 
reflection. 

In  this  way  two  haploscopic  images,  one  close  above  the 
other,  are  placed  in  the  central  field.  The  right  and  left 
components  of  each  of  these  can  be  either  synchronously  or 
alternately  compounded  in  each.  The  foveal  gaze  can  be 
turned  from  one  to  the  other  of  them  when  and  as  often  as 
the  observer  desires,  and  in  the  fraction  of  a  second  by  a  slight, 
/'.  e.  less  than  3°,  movement  of  the  eyeballs.  The  comparison 
thus  instituted  is  facile  and  sure. 


A.     SYMMETRICAL   FLICKER 

With  the  apparatus  thus  arranged  various  binocular  com- 
binations can  be  investigated  and  compared  either  one  with 
another  or  with  uniocular  images. 

As  shown  above,  the  apparatus  allows  of  similar  images 
being  thrown  on  strictly  and  fully  conjugate  points  of  the 
two  retinae,  either  synchronously  right  and  left  or  alternately 
right  and  left,  with  a  time  accuracy  not  less  than  .0006"  for 
the  slowest  rates  of  intermission,  and  not  less  than  .0001''  for  the 
highest.  The  first  comparison  made  (Experiment  i)  may  be 
to  observe  if  there  is  any  difference  between  the  rates  of  inter- 
mittence  for  just  perceptible  flicker  in  two  binocular  images, 
one  made  with  synchronous  right  and  left  illuminations,  the 
other  with  alternate  right  and  left  illuminations.     This  arrange- 


I 


SYMMETRICAL   BINOCULAR   FLICKER        363 


ment  is  expressed    graphically  by  the   accompanying  diagram 
(Fig.  78). 

The  diagram  makes  the  lower  composite  image  the  "  syn- 
chronous "  one,  but  in  the  series  of  observations  the  "  synchro- 
nous "  is  sometimes  the  lower,  sometimes  the  upper,  and  the 
observer  is  not  informed  which  it  may  be.  The  observations 
may  be  made  on  the  transition  from  flickering  to  unflickering 
sensation,  or  conversely  on  the  transition  from  unflickering  to 


^LR 


Figure  78. 


© 


jv© 


flickering  sensation ;  the  observer  in  the  latter  has  a  more  neutral 
approach  to  the  critical  observation.  Compared  under  rates  of 
intermittence  giving  marked  flicker  in  both  images,  observers 
find  the  flicker  "  less  "  in  the  "  alternate  "  than  in  the  "  synchro- 
nous "  combination.  This  difference  at  the  lower  speeds  inclines 
the  observer  to  expect  that  complete  extinction  of  the  flicker 
will  disappear  the  more  readily  in  the  image  which  at  slow  inter- 
missions seems  to  flicker  the  less.  Judgment  is  therefore  best 
asked  under  conditions  in  which  both  images  start  perfectly 
free  from  flicker,  the  rate  of  intermittence  being  from  the  outset 
high  enough  to  exclude  flicker. 

The  judgment  then  given  is  almost  uniformly  that  there 
does  exist  a  very  small  difference  between  the  frequency  of 
intermittence  required  for  extinction  of  flicker  in  the  "  syn- 
chronous "  and  "  alternate  "  combinations  respectively.  In  the 
"  alternate  *'  combination  flicker  disappears  at  a  slightly  lower 
frequency  of  intermission  than  in  the  "  synchronous."  All 
observers  agree  that  directly  the  frequency  of  intermission 
extinguishes  flicker  in  both  the  discs  the  appearance  of  both 


364  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

is  indistinguishably  similar,  and  that  there  is  then  nothing  to 
choose  between  the  brightness  of  the  two. 

For  almost  all  persons  I  have  examined,  a  spot  intermit- 
tently illuminated  at  a  frequency  of  intermission  just  sufficient 
to  extinguish  flicker  in  it,  when  looked  at  with  one  eye  only, 
still  flickers  slightly  when  looked  at  with  both  eyes.  A  like 
phenomenon  is  noticed  by  most  observers  when  examined  by 
the  arrangement  (Experiment  2)  represented  by  Figure  79. 


^^l_^l_J   r 


Figure  79. 

The  binocular  arrangement,  then,  is  said  by  them  to  require 
a  slightly  higher  frequency  for  extinction  of  flicker  than  does 
the  uniocular.  Again,  if  under  a  frequency  of  intermission 
just  securing  extinction  of  flicker  in  either  of  the  component 
uniocular  images  separately,  one  of  these  images,  previously 
screened  off",  is  readmitted,  so  that  the  pair  act  together  with 
a  synchronous  arrangement  of  phase,  a  trace  of  flicker  appears 
at  once  in  the  binocular  image.  It  may  be  urged  that  this  is 
due  to  the  fresh  retinal  area  being  more  sensitive  to  flicker,  and 
it  is  true  that  the  flicker  so  introduced  tends  soon  to  become 
less,  but  a  residuum  of  the  phenomenon  seems  to  remain. 

Experiment  3.  Conversely,  under  the  arrangement  indicated  in 
Figure  80,  a  number  of  the  persons  examined,  but  not  all,  decide 
that  the  binocular  image  requires  for  extinction  of  flicker  a  slightly 
lower  frequency  of  intermittent  illumination  than  does  the  uniocular. 
Also,  a  number  of  these  persons,  though  not  all,  find  that  when  the 
"  alternate  right  and  left  "  combination  is  observed  under  a  frequency 
of  intermission  of  illumination  just  sufficient  to  extinguish  its  flicker, 


XJ  SYMMETRICAL   BINOCULAR   FLICKER         365 

R 

X 

P 


Figure  So. 


the  screening  out  of  one  of  the  component  uniocular  images  brings 
with  it  a  slight  appearance  of  flicker. 

From  these  observations  it  appears  that  similar  phases  of  flicker- 
ing illumination  if  timed  to  fall  coincidently  on  conjugate  retinal  areas 
do  very  slightly  reinforce  each  other  in  sensation,  and  if  timed  exactly 
alternately  do  very  slightly  mutually  reduce.  But  the  broad  outcome 
of  the  observations  is  that  so  far  from  bright  phases  at  one  eye  effacing 
dark  phases  at  the  corresponding  spot  of  the  other  eye,  there  is  hardly 
a  trace  of  any  such  interference.  To  judge  from  its  absence  of  influence 
on  the  flicker  rate,  the  dark  phase  incident  at  retinal  point  A'  does  not, 
as  regards  sensual  result,  modify  the  bright  phase  synchronously  inci- 
dent at  the  conjugal  retinal  point  A,  and  conversely.  If  the  brightness 
of  the  bright  phase  or  the  darkness  of  the  dark  phase  were  lessened  at 
A  by  A',  the  rate  of  frequency  of  stimulus  for  extinction  of  flicker  must 
fall.  But  except  in  minute  and  perhaps  equivocal  degree  it  does  not 
alter. 

As  far  as  sensual  effect  goes,  the  light  phases  at  the  one  eye 
practically  do  not,  therefore,  interfere  or  combine  with  the  co- 
incident dark  phases  at  the  other;  and  conversely.  Nor  do 
they,  in  the  alternate  left  and  right  arrangement,  add  themselves 
as  a  series  of  additional  stimuli  to  the  like  series  of  stimuli 
applied  at  the  other  eye.  If  they  did  the  revolution  rate  of 
the  cylindrical  shutter  required  for  extinction  of  flicker  in  the 
upper  binocular  range  LR,  Fig.  80,  would  fall  far  below  that 
required  for  extinction  in  the  uniocular.  This  it  does  not  do. 
It  does  not  fall  at  all,  apart  from  the  minute  difference  noted  by 
some  persons  as  mentioned  above.     A  similar  result  is  obtained 


366  SENSUAL   FUSION  [Lect. 

under  the,  in  some  ways  more  decisive,  conditions  (Experiment 
4)  represented  in  Fig.  81. 

With  this  arrangement  no  observer  in  my  experiments  has 
ever  with  certainty  detected  difference  between  the  uniocular 
and  binocular  images  in  regard  to  either  the  apparent  rate  of 


X 


Figure  81. 


the  flicker  when  moderately  coarse  or  the  rate  of  intermission 
required  for  flicker  extinction.  This  arrangement  (Fig.  81) 
seems  the  most  crucial  for  deciding  the  point.  In  the  "  alter- 
nate right  and  left "  arrangement  (Fig.  78,  LR,  upper  com- 
bination) the  instants  of  change  of  phase  falling  together  right 
or  left,  it  might  be  that  it  did  not  matter  as  regards  flicker 
sensation  whether  the  direction  of  change  was  from  light  to  dark 
or  dark  to  light;  the  rates  of  intermission  being  the  same  right 
and  left,  and  the  instants  of  their  incidence  being  synchronous, 
it  might  then  be  that  as  regards  flicker  the  arrangement  was 
only  tantamount  to  the  *'  synchronous  right  and  left "  arrange- 
ment (Fig.  78,  lower  combination)  or  to  the  uniocular  inter- 
mittence  of  the  same  rate.  The  arrangement  (Fig.  81)  avoids 
this  dilemma.  Moreover  it  avoids  both  the  minute  reinforce- 
ment and  the  minute  reduction  of  flicker  inherent,  according 
to  the  above  experience,  in  the  exactly  "  synchronous  "  and 
"  alternate "  arrangements.  It  may  be  termed  for  conven- 
ience of  reference  the  "  intermediate  "  arrangement.  The  physi- 
ological stimulation  it  delivers  to  the  conjugate  retina  is  by 
any  mode  of  count  delivered  at  twice  the  rate  of  delivery  for 
either  retina  considered  apart  from  its  fellow.      Yet  the  rate  of 


i^ 


SYMMETRICAL  BINOCULAR  FLICKER         367 


revolution  of  the  cylindrical  lantern  required  to  extinguish  flicker 
in  this  experiment  remains  for  the  binocular  image  the  same  as 
for  the  uniocular. 

There  arises  the  question  whether  we  may  regard  the  dark 
field  covering  the  area  correspondent  with  that  to  which  in  the 
other  retina  a  bright  image  is  presented,  as  non-existent  visually. 
That  assumption  has  been  made  above,  and  is  indicated  in  the 
diagrams  (Figs.  79,  80,  81).  In  them,  where  one  image  is  repre- 
sented as  uniocular,  the  conjugate  area  of  the  other  retina  is 
left  out  of  the  diagram  altogether,  as  though  the  latter  retina 
were  non-existent,  or  for  the  time  being  blind.  This  seems  per- 
missible, when  care  is  taken  to  insure  absence  of  all  detail  or 
contour  from  the  dark  field  presented  to  the  other  retina,  except 
for  the  one  component  of  the  compared  binocular  image.  When 
that  field  is  perfectly  void  of  other  contours,  and  unchanging 
and  borderless,  it  is  found  to  matter  little  what  depth  of  darkness 
it  has ;  it  may  be  a  shade  of  gray  or  even  a  fair  white,  without 
perceptibly  influencing  the  sensual  vibrations  given  by  the  flick- 
ering image  before  the  other  eye.  The  condition  seems  com- 
parable with  the  familiar  disability  to  see  the  dark  field  presented 
to  one  closed  eye,  when  with  the  other  eye  the  observer  regards 
a  detailed  image.^^  For  these  reasons  the  visual  image  result- 
ing from  the  presentation  of  the  bright  disc  to  one  eye  only, 
as  in  the  arrangements  shown  by  Figs.  79,  80,  81,  was  regarded 
as  being  a  truly  uniocular  product,  uncomplicated  by  any  com- 
ponent from  the  other  retina.  The  corresponding  area  of  this 
latter  was  considered  as  for  the  time  being  out  of  action  as 
regards  sense,  not  so  much  by  darkness  as  by  virtue  of  border- 
less void  homogeneity  of  field,  —  as  when  eye-closure  affords 
visual  rest.  Under  this  blankness  the  "  retinal  points  "  become 
unhitched  from  the  running  machinery  of  consciousness,  if — 
and  this  is  essential — the  "corresponding"  retinal  area  be  con- 
currently under  stimulation  by  a  defined  image.  McDougall's  ^32 
principle  of  competition  for  energy  between  associate  neurones 
seems  at  work  here,  for  with  both  eyes  shut  the  dark  blankness 
of  eye-closure  does  become  visible.  Even  with  one  eye  open, 
if  its  field  be  undetailed  and   homogeneous,   glimpses  of  the 


368  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

"  Eigenschwarz  "  of  a  closed  eye  become  obtainable  (Purkinje, 
Volkmann,  E.  Hering). 

The  accurately  converse  stimulation  of  the  twin  retinal  areas 
might  be  expected  to  give  some  interference  of  the  flicker  sen- 
sations so  generated.  But  the  experimental  evidence  indicates 
absence  (practically  entire)  of  any  interference  between  the  flicker 
processes  so  initiated.  The  right  and  left  "  corresponding 
retino-cerebral  points  "  do  not  when  tested  by  flicker  reactions 
behave  as  though  combined  or  conjugate  to  a  single  mechanism. 
Their  sensual  reactions  retain  individuality  as  regards  time-rela- 
tions even  when  completely  confluent  as  judged  by  reference  to 
visual  space. 

B.    ASYMMETRICAL   FLICKER 

In  the  foregoing  experiments  the  flicker  sensations  of  "  cor- 
responding" areas  of  the  two  retinae  appear  (almost  entirely) 
without  influence  one  upon  another.  But  in  other  experiments 
the  flicker  test  reveals  very  considerable  mutual  influence  between 
reactions  initiated  at  the  corresponding  areas. 


Figure  'ii. 


Suppose  (Experiment  5)  two  binocular  images  LR  and  \p 
similarly  combined  from  similar  uniocular  components,  all  indi- 
vidually equal  in  brightness  and  in  intermission  frequency. 
Suppose  that  of  the  components  of  one  pair  (X/s)  one  (/))  be 
replaced  (Fig.  82)  by  an  intermittent  uniocular  image  (/)'),  of 
the  same  physical  brightness  as  that  giving  the  visual  image  /o, 
but   of  considerably  higher  intermission  frequency.     In  p^  all 


:]       ASYMMETRICAL   BINOCULAR   FLICKER         369 

licker  will  disappear  at  slower  speeds  of  revolution  of  the  lan- 
tern than  those  required  to  extinguish  flicker  in  L  or  R  or  \. 
'ig.  82  represents  the  arrangement. 

The  frequency  of  intermission  required  to  extinguish  flicker 
Xp  is  then  found  to  be  much  lower  than  the  frequency  required 
►r  extinction  of  flicker  in  LR,  or  in  L  or  R  or  \  separately, 
'hus  the  frequency  for  extinction  of  flicker  in  Xp  was  found 
observer  H.  H.)  to  average  52.2  phases  per  second  as  against 
I1.9  phases  per  second  for  LR,  or  for  L,  R,  or  X  separately. 

Screening  image  p'  out  of  the  binocular  combination  Xp', 
when  the  frequency  of  intermission  was  just  high  enough  to 
free  the  Xp'  image  from  flicker,  at  once  brought  flicker  into  it ; 
this  disappeared  immediately  image  p'  was  readmitted  to  the 
combination. 

In  this  instance  the  intensity  chosen  for  the  steady  illumina- 
tion of  the  conjugate  area  was  equal  to  that  employed  for  the 


Figure  83. 

uniocular  flickering  image.  The  duration  of  the  light  phases 
and  the  dark  per  revolution  of  the  lantern  was  equal,  and  the 
light  and  dark  phases  of  the  same  intensity  in  both.  But  the 
phenomenon  obtains  also  when  the  steady  uniocular  image  is 
less  bright  (Experiment  6,  Fig.  83)  or  more  bright  (Experi- 
ment 7,  Fig.  84)  than  the  flickering  uniocular  with  which  it  is 
combined.     The  following  example  illustrates  this. 

Experiment  6.  In  the  balanced  pair  of  binocular  images  LR  and 
Xp  made  of  carefully  equalized  intermittent  uniocular  images  L,  R,  X, 
and  p  the  uniocular  image  p  was  replaced  by  one  p'  of  five  times  greater 

24 


370  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

rapidity  of  intermission  and  giving  a  steady  image  of  only  ^  the  bright- 
ness of  the  images  L,  R,  X,  and  p  when  steady.  The  frequency  of  intermis- 
sion required  to  extinguish  flicker  in  the  binocular  image  kp'  (Fig.  83) 
was  then  found  to  be  72.1  phases  per  second,  whereas  in  LR,  and  in 
A,  L  and  R  separately,  it  was  75.5  phases  per  second  as  it  had  been  in 
the  previous.  The  steady  sensation  from  image  p'  therefore  damped 
the  vibration  of  the  flickering  sensation  from  the  conjugate  spot  under 
image  A.  by  an  amount  represented  by  3.4  phases  per  second. 


/''l_i_i_l_l_J 

Figure  84. 

Experiment  7  (Fig.  84)  illustrates  an  observation  in  which  for  the 
image  p  in  the  binocular  combination  \p  an  image  p'  was  substituted 
of  three  times  higher  frequency  of  intermission  and  giving  a  steady  image 
of  one  fifth  greater  brightness  than  the  image  L,  R,  and  A.  when  steady. 
It  was  then  found  that  the  frequency  of  intermission  required  to  extin- 
guish flicker  in  the  binocular  image  Ap'  (Fig.  84)  was  57.8  alternate 
equal  phases  (of  A)  per  second.  Whereas  in  LR,  and  in  A,  L,  and  R, 
taken  separately,  the  number  of  such  phases  required  was  63.6  per 
second. 

The  image  Xp'  was  distinctly  brighter  visually  than  was  LR, 
or  any  of  the  uniocular  images  A,  L,  and  R. 

These  observations  show,  as  do  the  observations  represented 
by  Fig.  82,  that  it  is  not  merely  the  reduction  of  brightness  in  the 
combined  image  \p  in  the  arrangement  shown  by  Fig.  83  that 
lessens  the  flicker  in  the  latter.  In  fact,  the  observations  on  the 
plan  illustrated  by  Fig.  84  we  have  the,  for  flicker  photometry, 
interesting  case  of  a  brighter  intermittently  illuminated  surface 
flickering  less  than  a  duller  one. 

Here  the  conditions  of  experiment  suggest  that  the  addition 


X]  UNIOCULAR  AND   BINOCULAR  371 

of  the  steady  brightness  at  one  eye  to  the  dark  phase  of  the 
intermittent  at  the  '*  corresponding  spot  "  lighten  the  latter,  and 
its  addition  to  the  phase  of  equal  brightness  with  it  leave  that 
practically  unaltered. 

That  might  be  evidence  of  mutual  interference  between 
purely  physiological  processes  initiated  at  the  corresponding 
spots  of  the  right  and  left  retinae.  But,  on  the  other  hand,  the 
result  at  once  suggests  that  the  binocular  product  from  a  uni- 
ocular  flickering  and  a  uniocular  unflickering  image  arises  by  a 
synthetic  process  akin  to  that  which  produces  from  a  pair  of 
individual  uniocular  brightnesses  a  binocular  brightness  near 
the  arithmetic  mean  of  the  brightness  of  the  two  components. 
The  rule  of  combination  exemplified  by  these  latter  finds  little 
solution  by  appeal  to  summation  or  interference  of  retinal  and 
purely  physiological  processes. 

Moreover,  the  supposition  that  the  sensual  reaction  caused 
by  a  steady  image  acting  at  one  of  the  pair  of  "  corresponding" 
areas,  is  interfering  with  or  combining  with  the  individual  phases 
of  reaction  to  the  intermittent  image  at  the  fellow  area,  is  exactly 
the  supposition  that  the  observations  dealing  with  symmetrical 
flicker  show  to  be  untenable. 


UNIOCULAR  AND   BINOCULAR   COMPARISONS 

With  intermittent  lights  throughout  a  wide  range  of  ordinary 
intensities  Talbot's  ^*  law  is  unimpeachable  for  the  single  eye ; 
and  also  for  the  two  eyes  if  employed  together  under,  as  is  usual, 
arrangements  practically  equivalent  to  the  "  simultaneous  "  right- 
left  method  for  "  symmetrical  flicker."  It  is  interesting  to  dis- 
cover how  far  the  double  retina  will  still  observe  Talbot's  law 
when  subjected  to  treatment  such  that,  if  the  retina  did  then  ob- 
serve the  law,  would  indicate  its  integration  to  a  functionally 
single  retina.  In  other  words,  under  a  rapidly  repeated  stimu- 
lus, when  one  incidence  of  that  stimulus  has  acted  on  a  retinal 
point  the  question  is:  how  far  is  it  the  same  thing  for  visual 
brightness,  whether  the  next  incidence  be  upon  the  same  retinal 
point  or  upon  the  twin  point  in  the  other  retina?     How  far  can 


372  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

the  double  retina,  when  functioning  for  singleness  of  perception 
in  binocular  vision,  be  considered  as  functionally  combined  to 
a  single  retina,  and  how  far  does  it  then  react  as  does  a  single 
retina,  if  examined  for  Talbot's  law? 

The  "  alternate  left-right  arrangement"  (Experiment  i,  LR) 
supplies  the  required  method  of  stimulation.  With  speeds 
of  revolution  of  the  lantern  too  high  to  allow  flickering,  the 
binocular  image  LR  (Fig.  79)  is  seen  to  appear  of  brightness 
equal  to  X/?,  and  with  the  uniocular  images  \  or  p  taken  singly. 
Therefore  in  the  above  sense,  Talbot's  law  not  only  does  not  hold 
for  the  double  retina  considered  as  functionally  single,  but  no 
trace  of  observance  of  the  law  is  detectable.  The  two  corres- 
ponding points  are  therefore  in  this  respect  not  integrated  to 
a  single  retinal  surface. 

It  was  often  noted  that  with  all  four  lantern  images  of  equal 
luminosity,  using  intermission  frequencies  too  rapid  to  allow 
flicker,  the  brightness  of  the  binocular  combination  of  any  two 
did  not  distinctly  exceed  that  of  the  uniocular.  In  certain  in- 
stances the  binocular  combination  did  appear  just  distinctly  the 
brighter.  This  was  for  instance  the  case  when  of  the  four  lan- 
tern images  the  two  on  the  same  horizontal  level  were  combined 
by  simple  convergence.  This  excess  of  brightness  is  the  well- 
known  phenomenon  examined  by  Jurin,^  Harris,^  Fechner,^^ 
Aubert,^^  Valerius,^^  and  others.  But  there  occurred  frequent 
instances  in  which  no  excess  was  observed  in  the  brightness  of 
binocular  combinations  over  that  of  their  carefully  balanced 
uniocular  components.  In  these  observations  the  brightness  of 
the  physical  images  is  however  always  much  above  the  threshold 
of  the  light  adapted  eye;  and  I  have  not  made  systematic  obser- 
vations with  the  eye  dark-adapted.  To  obtain  good  conditions 
for  comparison  of  the  brightness  of  the  binocular  and  uniocular 
images  the  following  arrangement  can  be  employed. 

Experiment  8.  Two  images  LR  and  \p\  are  placed  in  the  visual 
field  for  mutual  comparison.  LR  is  composed  of  left-eye  and  right-eye 
equal  and  corresponding  disc-shaped  images  as  in  previous  experiments. 
X/)^  is  composed  of  a  left-eye  image  similar  to  L  and  R  except  that  it 
lies  just  above  or  below  them  in  the  visual  field.     With  X's  right  half 


X]      TALBOT'S  LAW  AND  BINOCULAR  RETINA  "373 

is  combined  the  image  of  the  right  half  of  a  lantern  image  similar  again 
[to  the  others,  except  that  its  left  half  is  screened  absolutely  off  into  the 
)lank  undetailed  darkness  of  the  general  field.  When  this  is  done  the 
two  opposite  visual  images  LR  and  Ap^  regarded  under  perfectly  steady 
ocular  fixation  are  stable,  and  no  difference  of  brightness  is  discernible 
between  them.  Moreover  no  join  is  seen  between  the  halves  of  Ap^  and 
no  difference  of  brightness  between  the  halves.  After  prolonged  in- 
spection of  them  rivalry  becomes  troublesome ;  but  a  judgment  can  be 
clearly  arrived  at  before  that  happens. 

In  this   experiment  it  might   possibly  be   that   equality  of 
brightness  between  the  halves  of  \pi  is  due  to  image  pi  not 


V.0 


Exp.  9. 

Figure  85. 


really  being  in  consciousness  at  all  during  the  comparison.  The 
image  might  possibly  lapse  under  competition  with  the  partly 
dissimilar  correspondingly  placed  left-eye  image  \.  Experi- 
ments carried  out  by  W.  McDougall^^  give  validity  to  such 
possible  objection.  The  perceptibility  of  the  horizontal  bar  in 
the  right  half  of  the  image  X/oi  is  guarantee  however  that  at 
least  part  of  the  uniocular  image  pi  is  present.  But  to  ascer- 
tain more  surely  whether  image  pi  is  really  during  the  visual 
equation  co-operating  in  consciousness  with  X  the  following 
further  arrangement  can  be  employed. 

Experiment^  (Fig.  85).  With  the  revolving  lantern  so  arranged 
that  images  L,  R,  A,  and  pi  are  all  of  equal  brightness  when  steady  and 
unflickering,  pi  is  given  at  a  lesser  frequency  of  intermission,  so  as  to 
flicker  while  the  others  do  not.  A  speed  of  revolution  of  lantern  is  then 
used  at  which  just  a  trace  of  flicker  is  perceptible  in  pi  when  binocularly 


374  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

combined  with  A.  The  equation  LR  =  Ap^  is  then  found  to  hold  while 
flicker  is  still  just  traceable  in  the  right  half  of  Ap^.  There  is  then  no 
join  seen  between  the  halves  of  kp^  nor  any  difference  between  the 
brightness  of  the  halves.  So  long  as  ocular  fixation  is  steady  no  rivalry 
disturbs  the  observation. 

In  this  case  there  can,  I  think,  be  no  question  but  that  the 
one  half  of  \pi  is  truly  binocular,  for  the  trace  of  flicker  is 
perceptible  during  the  actual  performance  of  the  comparison. 
Yet  no  difference  of  brightness  is  perceived  betv^een  LR  and 
\pi,  and  the  two  lateral  halves  of  Xpi  compared  together 
seem  of  like  brightness. 

Even  when  the  binocular  image  does  show  the  well-known 
slight  excess  of  brightness  over  its  uniocular  components  it, 
under  some  conditions  (v.  supra,  "  alternate "  arrangement), 
flickers  no  more  or  even  less  than  they. 

It  is  doubtful  therefore  to  me  whether  the  slight  excess  in 
brightness  of  the  binocular  image  over  its  two  equal  uniocular 
components  is  really  explicable  as  summation  of  the  intensities 
of  the  reactions  at  the  corresponding  spots  of  the  two  retinae. 
Valerius  ^2  measured  the  increase  to  be  one  fifteenth  of  the 
brightness  of  the  uniocular  image.  Aubert's^^  diagram  gives  it 
as  less  than  one  thirtieth.  Aubert  says  it  is  not  perceptible 
with  brightness  greater  than  that  of  white  paper  in  diffuse  day- 
light indoors.®^* 

In  certain  modes  of  experiment  a  uniocular  image  used  as 
a  standard  for  comparison  might  itself  be  suffering  some  reduc- 
tion in  brightness  owing  to  slight  combination  with  the  dark 
field  presented  concurrently  at  the  corresponding  retinal  area. 
But  "rivalry"  should  reveal  such  influence.  A  better  definition 
and  greater  vividness  of  detail  assured  by  better  accommodation 
and  convergence  under  binocular  regard,  might  possibly  give 
an  appearance  of  greater  brilliance  and  intensity.  But  these  are 
only  suggestions. 

I  conclude  that,  with  ordinary  intensities  of  illumination, 
although  a  binocular  image  does  sometimes  appear  of  slightly 
greater  visual  brightness  than  either  of  two  similar  uniocular  images 
composing  it,  more  often  it  has  a  visual  brightness  not  perceptibly 


X]   UNIOCULAR  AND  BINOCULAR  BRIGHTNESS    375 

different  from  that  of  either  of  its  two  co-equal  uniocuiar  com- 
ponents. The  case  then  falls  within  a  general  rule  regarding  binoc- 
ular brightness  attested  by  all  observations  I  have  on  that  subject. 
A  binocular  brightftess  compared  with  its  uniocuiar  components  is  of 
value  not  greater  than  the  greater  of  those ^  nor  less  than  the  lesser 
of  them;  when  free  from  oscillations  of  rivalry  its  value  is 
somewhat y  but  not  far ^  above  the  arithmetic  mean  of  the  values 
of  the  two  uniocuiar  components  as  expressed  by  the  measures  of  the 
physical  stimuli  yielding  them. 

The  various  combinations  cited  in  Experiments  2,  3,  4,  5,  6, 
7,  and  8  have  all,  when  steady  and  unflickering,  given  bright- 
nesses illustrating  the  above  rule.     Other  illustrations  are 

X  =  1000,  p  —    250,  \p  =    680, 

X=iooo,  /)=    350.  X/>=    750, 

X=iooo,  p=    550,  \p=    83s, 

X  =  1000,  p  —    750,  \p  —    920, 

X  =  1000,  p  =  1000,  \p  =  ICXX). 

But  I  have  not  worked  with  combinations  where  the  physical 
luminosity  of  one  uniocuiar  component  has  been  less  than  y"^th 
the  physical  luminosity  of  the  other.  It  was  near  this  limit  that 
Aubert,  and  just  beyond  it  that  Fechner,  noted  decline  of  the 
darkening  effect  of  the  darker  component.  In  my  own  few 
observations  beyond  that  point  the  oscillations  of  rivalry  have 
made  judgment  difficult.  The  more  manageable  examples  are 
but  demonstrations  of  "  Fechner's  paradox,"  and  fall  under  the 
above  general  rule.  Hering^  has  suggested  that  rivalry  is 
really  occurring  even  with  similar  and  right  and  left  uniocuiar 
images;  he  says  these  react  according  to  a  law  of  "comple- 
mental  shares,"  and  offers  a  theory,  such  as  the  name  he  gives 
implies,  in  explanation  of  the  phenomenon.  My  Experiment  9 
seems  to  offer  difficulty  to  such  a  view. 

Binocular  combination  of  a  less  bright  image  with  a  more 
bright  gives  a  visual  image  of  less  brightness  than  the  latter 
(as  stated  in  the  rule  above).  But  the  application  of  the  less 
bright  physical  image  to  the  same  uniocuiar  area  as  the  more 
bright  gives  a  visual  image  of  brightness  greater  than  either. 


376  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

As  above  described,  a  steady  image  presented  on  an  area  of 
one  retina  "  damps  "  the  flicker  of  a  flickering  image  concur- 
rently presented  at  the  corresponding  area  of  the  other.  A 
steady  image  actually  physically  superposed  on  the  same  retinal 
area  as  a  flickering  one  also  reduces  the  latter's  flicker;  this 
latter  is  of  course  in  accordance  with  Weber's  law.  The  modes 
of  interference  seem  incomparably  different  in  the  two  cases  ; 
and  experiment  shows  that  the  two  interferences  are  often  quite 
of  different  value. 

Experiment  lo  A,  Binocular  fusion  of  R  and  L  gives  flicker  ex- 
tinction at  65.5  phases  per  second. 

Physical  fusion  of  R  and  L  gives  flicker  extinction  at  59.4  phases 
per  second.  Observer  G.  C. 

Binocular  fusion  of  R  and  L  gives  flicker  extinction  at  106.6  phases 
per  second. 

Physical  fusion  of  R  and  L  gives  flicker  extinction  at  100.3  phases 
per  second. 

R  separately  gives  flicker  extinction  at  113.3  phases  per  second. 

Observer  R.  S.  W. 

Finally,  to  touch  on  "  predominance  of  contours."  Its  facts, 
established  by  so  many  workers,  are  among  the  most  signifi- 
cant concerning  the  difference  between  binocular  and  uniocular 
fusion  of  visual  reactions.  I  will  merely  give  one  illustration 
which  seems  specially  instructive  for  the  point  before  us. 

Experiment  11,  A  steady  unflickering  disc-shaped  image  L  is  pres- 
ent to  the  left  eye  :  across  the  disc  is  a  narrow  dark  line.  An  image  R 
of  similar  size  and  shape  but  without  the  dark  line  is  presented  to  the 
corresponding  area  of  the  right  eye.  If  the  luminosity  of  L  is  progres- 
sively diminished,  a  value  of  luminosity  is  reached  at  which  its  cross- 
line,  though  visible  when  L  alone  is  observed  {e.  g.  right  eye  closed) 
is  lost  or  uncertain  in  the  binocular  image  RL.  This  reduction  of  the 
luminosity  of  L  much  exceeds  the  reduction  at  which  its  cross-line  is 
lost  when  image  R  is  concurrently  thrown  on  the  same  area  of  the 
same  retina,  /.  e.  left  retina.  Thus,  in  one  experiment  the  diminution 
of  luminosity  of  L  required  for  loss  of  the  cross-line  under  the  physical 
superposition  of  R  and  L  on  the  same  retina  was  84  per  cent,  while 
the  diminution  of  luminosity  of  L  required  for  loss  (or  great  uncer- 
tainty)  of  the  line  in  the  binocular  image  was  96  per  cent 


X]      UNIOCULAR  AND  BINOCULAR  CONTRAST    377 

Not  only  the  ease,  but  the  mode  of  disappearance  of  the 
cross-line,  is  significantly  different  in  the  two  cases.  In  the 
"  physical  superposition "  the  dark  line  became  gradually 
thinner  and  fainter,  and  finally  imperceptible,  as  the  image  L 
is  lessened  in  luminosity.  In  the  case  of  binocular  fusion  the 
dark  line  oscillates  out  of  and  back  into  sensation  more  and 
more,  the  disappearances  predominating  more  and  more  as  the 
darkening  of  L  proceeds.  At  a  reduction  of  84  per  cent  of 
the  luminosity  of  L  the  cross-line  was  steady,  dark,  and  sharp 
in  the  binocular  image. 

Our  aim  has  been  information  as  to  the  nature  of  the  con- 
junction between  the  uniocular  components  in  certain  simple 
binocular  sensations.  The  question  concerns  the  nature  of  the 
tie  between  "corresponding  retinal  points,"  meaning  by  "retinal 
point"  the  retino-cerebral  apparatus  engaged  in  elaborating  a 
sensation  in  response  to  excitation  of  a  unit  area  of  retinal 
surface. 

That  a  sensation  initiated  from  corresponding  retinal  points 
is  commonly  referred  without  ambiguity  to  a  single  locus  in 
visual  space  has  often  been  regarded  (Newton,^  Wollaston, ^* 
Rohault,^  Joh.  Miiller^^)  as  evidence  of  community  of  the  nerve 
apparatus  belonging  to  the  paired  retinal  points.  Their  visual 
image  appears  single.  Wollaston  supposed  the  twin  points 
attached  to  one  and  the  same  nerve-fibre,  which  bifurcated  at 
the  chiasma.  Rohault  and  Miiller  supposed  the  points  to  be 
served  by  twin  fibres  "  from  one  and  the  same  ganglion-cell  in 
the  cerebral  substance."  Later  (cf.  Aubert^),  the  visual  single- 
ness, spatial  fusion  of  right  and  left  impressions  to  a  single  per- 
ception, was  taken  to  mean  confluence  of  the  nerve-processes, 
started  in  right  and  left  retinae  respectively,  to  "  a  single 
common  centre  or  point  of  the  sensorium."  The  discovery 
later  still  that  the  fibre-tracts  from  corresponding  halves  of  the 
retinae  both  go  to  the  occipital  region  of  one  and  the  same 
hemisphere  has  also  been  inferred  to  mean  a  spatially  conjoint 
visual  sensorium  common  to  both  retinae  {e.  g.  Ramon-y-Cajal, 
Schafer).     But  in  such  questions  the  inferences  obtainable  from 


378  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

mere  anatomical  features  are  equivocal  and  often  remote  in 
bearing.  Were  there  to  exist  such  a  common  mechanism  situ- 
ate as  a  unit  at  conjunction  of  the  two  convergent  systems  and 
were  phases  of  excitement  timed  so  to  arrive  from  one  retina  as 
exactly  to  fill  pauses  between  excitations  transmitted  from  the 
other,  then  there  should  be  evidence  of  this  in  the  time-relations 
of  the  phenomena  induced.  The  state  of  excitement  should  tend 
to  be  maintained  across  periods  that  would  otherwise  checker  it 
as  pauses. 

The  retino-cerebral  apparatus  may  be  regarded  as  a  structure 
of  linked  branching  nerve-elements  forming  a  system  which  ex- 
pands as  traced  centrally  from  the  retinal  surface.  It  may  be 
figured  as  a  tree,  with  its  stem  at  the  retina  and  an  arborization 
spreading  into  the  brain,  its  ramifications  there  penetrating  a 
vast  cerebral  field,  interlacing  with  others  in  a  cerebral  forest 
composed  of  nervous  arborizations.  The  simile  fails,  because  in 
the  nervous  forest  the  arborizations  make  functional  union  one 
with  another.  Is  the  fusion  of  the  perceptions  adjunct  to  paired 
"corresponding  points"  the  outcome  of  a  close  concrescence  of 
their  neuronic  or  neuro-fibrillar  arborizations,  making  of  them 
practically  a  single  upgrowth  common  to  twin  (right  and  left) 
stems  rooted  in  the  corresponding  retinal  units?  If  so,  how  low 
down,  how  close  to  their  origin,  are  the  twin  systems  grafted 
together,  giving  structural  community  to  all  the  superstructure? 

In  the  chain  of  nerve-elements  attached  to  a  sense-organ  we 
infer  in  general  that  to  the  activities  of  the  most  peripheral  links 
per  se  psychical  events  are  not  adjunct.  Psychical  processes, 
beginning  with  least  complex  and  ascending  toward  develop- 
ment through  many  grades,  attach  to  the  chain  in  such  a  way 
that  for  the  simplest  only  the  more  peripheral  portions  of  the 
chain  need  be  connected  with  the  sense-organ,  while  for  the 
more  complex  the  central  portions  in  addition  become  more 
extensively  involved.  But  in  the  higher  reactions  of  definite 
physical  aspect,  e.  g.  sense-perceptions,  the  lower  apsychic  and 
less  definitely  psychic  activities  are  also  implicate.  Where 
from  two  sense-organs,  e.  g.  two  units  of  retinal  surface,  the 
two  nerve-chain  arborizations  are  mutually  connected,  so  that 


X]  BINOCULAR  FUSION  379 

the  lower  activities  of  the  one  affect  by  low-level  side  connections 
the  elements  forming  the  other,  there  analysis  must  fail  to  distin- 
guish in  the  full  reaction  what  higher  components  may  be  sepa- 
rately referable  to  one  only  of  the  two  individual  chains.  The 
processes  apsychic,  or  so  indefinitely  psychic  as  to  baffle  intro- 
spection, at  root  of  those  amenable  to  introspection,  must  by 
their  coalescence  defeat  attempt  to  trace  the  final  psychical 
product  to  either  of  its  two  possible  sources,  so  long  as  both 
sources  are  open  for  its  originatfon. 

Were  the  nervous  reactions  initiated  at  twin  points  of  the  ret- 
inae early  in  its  path  along  the  retino-cerebral  nerve-chain,  to 
enter  mechanisms  common  to  both,  there  must,  under  "  alter- 
nate "  or  "synchronous"  right-left  arrangement  of  stimuli  (Fig. 
78),  be  interference,  algebraic  summation,  etc.,  a  coalescence  of 
events  which,  though  apsychical  in  itself,  would  involve  subse- 
quent confusion  together  of  the  sense-reactions  of  the  two  eyes. 
A  state  of  things  wholly  different  from  this  is  revealed  in  the 
results  of  our  experiments.  And  it  would  amount  to  the  same 
thing  whether  two  quickly  successive  flashes  of  a  light  fell  both  on 
one  and  the  same  member  of  a  pair  of  "  corresponding  points," 
or  whether  the  first  fell  upon  one  member,  the  second  upon  the 
other.  But  the  experiments  show  that  the  effect  in  the  two 
cases  is  widely  different.  Talbot's  law  is  not  applicable  to  the 
double  retina,  that  is,  to  the  two  retinae  functioning  together  in 
binocular  vision.  The  experimental  results  go  to  disapprove 
the  existence  of  any  such  fusion  or  interference  between  the 
apsychical  or  even  the  subperceptual  events  arising  from  corre- 
sponding retinal  points.  At  most  they  indicate  hardly  discernible 
traces  of  such  interference  (Experiment  i).  They  indicate,  on 
the  contrary,  that  such  simple  forms  of  binocular  perception  as 
have  been  dealt  with  here  are  themselves  fusions  of  elaborated 
uniocular  sensations.  Since  left  and  right  end-results  emerge 
pure,  "  hybridization "  has  not  mixed  the  early  stages  in  their 
evolution. 

But  the  difference  between  the  modes  of  stimulation  left  and 
right  is  a  difference  that,  although  it  should  be  potent  if  the  left 
and  nght  physiological  machinery  were  conjoined  to  unity,  should 


38o  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

constitute  no  difference  when  left  stimulation  is  compared  with 
right  stimulation  by  the  perceptual  product  which  each  yields. 
The  left  eye  and  right  eye  flickering  visual  images^  each  viewed 
singly,  do  NOT  (apart  from  the  faint  cross-line  for  recognition) 
differ  to  introspection.  If  the  sensations  derived  from  the  left 
eye  and  right  eye  respectively  appear  under  introspection  indis- 
tinguishably  alike,  what  ground  is  there  for  mutual  interference 
between  them?  It  is  much  as  though,  of  the  left  and  right  lan- 
tern images  each  were  seen  by  one  of  two  observers,  with  similar 
vision,  and  as  though  the  minds  of  the  two  observers  were  com- 
bined to  a  single  mind.  It  may  be  recalled  that  binocular  unifi- 
cation of  images,  as  we  possess  it,  seems  a  comparatively  late 
achievement  of  phylogenetic  evolution. 

When  the  visual  product  of  the  two  retinae  is  thus  regarded 
it  is  not  surprising  that  Talbot's  law  fails  for  the  binocular  cyclo- 
pean  retina.  It  fails  because  the  binocular  sensation  is  a  fusion 
of  uniocular  sensation  and  from  no  two  similar  sensations  can  a 
resultant  sensation  be  compounded  different  from  its  com- 
ponents. Were  Talbot's  law  to  hold  in  the  above  sense  for  the 
binocular  retina  there  would,  under  the  "  alternate  left-right 
arrangement"  (symmetrical  flicker),  at  rates  of  intermission  too 
high  for  flicker,  result  from  an  image  L  of  brightness  x,  and  an 
image  R  of  similar  brightness  x,  a  combined  image  LR  of 
brightness  x  +  x,  the  value  of  the  summed  brightness  being  in 
accord  with  the  Weber-Fechner  rule  of  summation  of  sensual 
intensities.  But,  as  shown,  not  only  does  this  summation  not 
occur,  but  nothing  like  it  occurs.  The  binocular  result  most 
often  does  not  perceptibly  differ  from  either  of  its  two  co-equal 
components. 

But  the  experiments  with  uniocular  components  dissimilarly 
flickering,  and  with  flickering  components  concurrent  with 
steady  components,  do  evidence  (unlike  the  other  experiments) 
interference  between  the  two  eyes.  This  result  might  be  inter- 
preted as  the  outcome  of  community  of  the  physiological 
mechanisms  attaching  to  the  paired  "  corresponding  retinal 
points."  But  the  other  experiments  negative  the  existence  of 
this   community.     And    the   explanation  just   offered   for   the 


X]  BINOCULAR  FUSION  381 

absence  of  interference  in  the  other  experiments  will  account  for 
the  presence  of  interference  in  these.  From  two  components 
perceptibly  differing  between  themselves  in  regard  to  some 
quality  {.e.g.  flicker)  a  single  combined  sensual  quality  is  ob- 
tained, intermediate  between  that  of  the  two  components  taken 
singly.  If  the  perceptible  difference,  e.  g.  in  flicker,  between  the 
components  is  wide,  the  fusion  is  liable  to  phasic  oscillations  of 
predominance  of  one  or  other  component.  Where  the  difference 
in  flicker  is  wide,  such  "  rivalry  "  between  the  right  and  left  com- 
ponents is  in  fact  not  unfrequently  seen.  One  component  may 
at  the  height  of  its  phase  be  alone  perceptible  at  the  focus  of 
attention,  the  other  component  being  inhibited  out  of  focal  atten- 
tion or  even  out  of  conscious  vision  altogether.  The  inference 
is  that  only  after  the  sensations  initiated  from  right  and  left 
"  corresponding  points  "  have  been  elaborated ^  and  have  reached  a 
dignity  and  definiteness  well  ameftable  to  introspection,  does  inter- 
ference between  the  reactions  of  the  two  {left  and  right)  eye-systems 
occur.  The  binocular  sensation  attained  seems  combined  from 
right  and  left  uniocular  sensations  elaborated  independently. 

And  in  harmony  with  this  view  stands  the  evidence  adduced 
for  the  rule  formulated  regarding  the  relation  of  binocular  to 
uniocular  brightness.  Further,  the  difference  between  the  sen- 
sual result  of  superposition  of  two  similar  images  upon  one  and 
the  same  area  of  a  single  retina,  and  upon  twin  areas  of  the  two 
retinae,  could  hardly  be  so  great  as  it  is,  did  apsychical  or  sub- 
sensual  reactions  underlying  "  brightness  "  combine  or  interfere 
in  the  two  retinal  systems.  The  binocular  combination  must  be 
a  synthesis  of  a  left-eye  with  a  right-eye  sensation.  Similarly, 
the  "  prevalence  of  contours  "  in  binocular  vision,  and  the  phe- 
nomena of  *'  retinal  rivalry,"  are  explicable  if  each  member  of  a 
pair  of  corresponding  points  yields  a  sensual  entity  which,  when 
not  widely  dissimilar  from  that  yielded  by  its  twin  point,  fuses 
with  that  to  a  binocular  sensation.  In  "  retinal  rivalry  "  we  have 
an  involuntarily  performed  analysis  of  this  sensual  bicompound. 
The  binocular  perception  in  that  case  breaks  down,  leaving 
phasic  periods  of  one  or  other  of  the  simpler  component  sensa- 
tions bare  to  inspection. 


382  SENSUAL   FUSION  [Lect. 

W.  McDougall,  2^^  in  applying  to  "  retinal  rivalry "  and 
"  prevalence  of  contours"  his  principle  of  competition  of  inter- 
related nerve-elements  for  energy,  also  argues  a  "  separateness 
of  the  visual  cortical  areas  for  the  two  eyes."  He  brings  for- 
ward striking  experiments  in  evidence  of  this.  In  one  of  these 
he  ^^  shows  that  an  after-image,  left  from  excitation  of  one 
retina,  is  more  strongly  revived  by  subsequent  weak  diffuse 
excitation  of  that  same  retina  than  of  its  fellow.  More  recently, 
in  experiments  proving  reinforcement  of  visual  sensations  by 
the  activity  of  the  ocular  muscles,  as  evidenced  by  after-image 
observations,  he  ^^^a  shows  that  activity  of  the  intrinsic  muscles 
of  an  eye  sends  up  to  the  brain  an  influence,  reinforcing  the 
activity  of  the  cerebro-retinal  tract  of  that  eye,  while  it  exerts 
no  such  effect  upon  the  corresponding  tract  of  the  other  eye,  or 
exerts  it  in  a  minor  degree  only.  With  this  separateness  of  the 
mechanisms,  wherein  are  produced  the  sensations  generated  in 
the  two  retinae,  our  results  by  a  different  line  of  experimentation 
accord  well. 

The  compounding  together  of  right  and  left  images  really 
nonidentical  but  not  widely  dissimilar,  is  (Panum,  Hering)  the 
basis  of  visual  "  relative  depth-perception."  The  compounding 
of  visual  images  partly  dissimilar  —  flickering  with  unflickering  — 
seems  a  simpler  case  in  the  same  category  of  synthetic  actions. 
In  our  flicker  experiments  the  visual  components  do  not  differ 
as  to  space-attributes,  and  their  combination  has  therefore  no 
resultant  differential  space-attribute.  But  the  synthesis  gives  in 
each  case  a  compromise  between  the  components  in  regard  to 
the  attribute  wherein  they  do  differ;  in  the  flicker  experiments, 
that  is  in  regard  to  the  sensual  steadiness  of  the  brightness. 
This  amounts  to  the  same  as  the  rule  formulated  above  for 
binocular  combination  of  brightness  of  different  intensities,  but 
steady. 

Our  experiments  show,  therefore,  that  during  binocular  regard 
of  an  objective  image  each  uniocular  mechanism  develops  inde- 
pendently a  sensual  image  of  considerable  completeness.  The 
singleness  of  the  binocular  perception  results  from  tmion  of  these 
elaborated  uniocular  sensations.     The  singlcfiess  is  therefore  the 


X]        VISUAL  FUSION   AND   NEURAL  UNION       383 

product  of  a  synthesis  that  works  with  already  elaborated  sen^ 
sations  contemporaneously  proceeding. 

The  cerebral  seats  of  right-eye  and  left-eye  visual  images 
are  thus  shown  to  be  separate.  Conductive  paths  no  doubt 
interconnect  them,  but  are  shown  to  be  unnecessary  for  visual 
unification  of  the  two  images.  The  unification  of  a  sensation  of 
composite  source  is  evidently  associated  with  a  neurone  arrange- 
ment different  from  that  which  obtains  in  the  synthesis  of  a 
reflex  movement  by  the  convergence  of  the  reflexes  of  allied 
arcs  upon  its  final  common  paths. 

Here  we  seem  to  have  therefore  contemporaneity  of  itself 
sufficing  for  sensual  synthesis,  without  necessarily  any  spatial 
fusion  of  the  neural  processes  or  mechanisms  involved,  i,  e. 
without  spatial  confluence  to  a  unit  apparatus.  As  mentioned 
above,  W.  McDougall's  experiments  on  after-images  lead  to  a 
like  conclusion.  The  foundation  of  new  correspondences  be- 
tween retinal  points  in  cases  of  squint  (Tschermak)  strengthen 
the  same  view.  McDougall  has  recently  well  summarized  the 
position.  But  it  is  one  not  generally  admitted  by  physiologists 
or  psychologists.  Ziehen  2*2a  pyrites :  "  Schon  physio) ogisch  ist 
die  Verschmeltzung  der  beiden  Netzhautbilder  dadurch  vor- 
bereitet  dass  die  Erregungen  welche  auf  den  linken  Halften 
beider  Netzhaute  auftreten,  vermoge  der  eigenthiimlichen  par- 
tiellen  Sehnerven-kreuzung  zusammen  in  die  rechte  Grosshirn 
hemisphare  gelangen,  und  umgekehrt."  And  this  was  the  view 
of  Joh.  Miiller  and  of  Aubert,  and  is  advanced  on  histological 
grounds  by  Ramon-y-Cajal. 

The  results  bear  also  on  the  production  of  sensual  reactions 
and  states  more  complex  than  those  of  the  examples  taken. 
Hartmann,  as  quoted  by  McDougall,  writes:  "Only  because 
one  part  of  my  brain  has  a  direct  communication  with  the  other 
is  the  consciousness  of  the  two  parts  unified.  Could  we  unite 
the  brains  of  two  human  beings  by  a  path  of  communication 
equivalent  to  cerebral  fibres  both  would  no  longer  have  two  but 
one  consciousness."  There  is  no  denying  the  extreme  impor- 
tance and  the  vast  actual  extent  of  the  spatial  conjunction  of 
cerebral  elements  by  conductive  channels  in  sensual  and  per- 


384  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

ceptual  reactions.  Yet  I  cannot  but  think  that  its  limitless 
postulation  leads  not  so  much  to  explanation  of  the  high  degree 
of  unity  of  the  individual  mind  as  to  an  ultimate  fallacy  which 
Professor  James  has  trenchantly  termed  that  of  "  the  pontifical 
cell."  Pure  conjunction  in  time  without  necessarily  cerebral 
conjunction  in  space  lies  at  the  root  of  the  solution  of  the  prob- 
lem of  the  unity  of  mind. 

Since  convergence  of  the  conductors  from  corresponding 
halves  of  the  retinae  to  the  same  field  of  brain-cortex  does  not 
signify  physiological  conjunction  of  right  and  left  sense-impres- 
sions, can  we  decipher  at  all  what  it  does  mean?  To  do  so  does 
not  seem  difficult,  and  displays  strikingly  the  different  value  and 
directness  of  spatial  union  of  conducting-paths  for  motor  synthe- 
sis and  for  psychical  respectively.  In  animals  with  overlapping 
visual  fields  the  lateral  movements  of  the  eyeball  have  a  mutual 
relation  different  from  the  ordinary  relations  of  the  movements 
of  a  unilaterally  placed  organ,  e.  g.  a  limb.  Especially  is  this 
the  case  where  the  overlap  of  the  visual  fields  is  extensive,  e.g. 
where  the  ocular  axes  are  parallel.  The  horizontal  movements 
of  each  eyeball  are  balanced  about  the  primary  line  of  vision  of 
the  globus  in  its  habitual  resting  attitude,  that  is,  in  man, 
straight  forward.  That  line  sensually,  as  shown  by  introspective 
experiment,  lies  in  the  median  sagittal  plane  of  the  head  (Her- 
ing).  Hence  the  term  '  Cyclopean  *  has  been  applied  to  the 
biunial  eye  of  human  binocular  vision.  Finding  the  median 
vertical  plane  of  sight  of  the  resting  eyeball  to  correspond  with 
the  median  sagittal  plane  of  the  body,  we  may  assume  that  the 
motor  reflexes  deal  with  the  eyeball  conformably  with  that; 
otherwise  there  would  be  disaccord  between  the  reflexes  and 
the  sensations.  Therefore  we  must  in  any  general  consideration 
of  the  taxis  of  the  lateral  movements  of  the  two  eyeballs  transfer 
in  thought  each  eyeball  from  its  own  actual  sagittal  plane  to 
the  median  sagittal  plane  of  the  head ;  and  this  latter  corre- 
sponds in  the  resting  position  with  the  sagittal  median  plane  of 
the  animal  as  a  whole. 

Each  lateral  muscle  of  the  eyeball  comes  therefore  to  bear 
to  the  median  plane  of  the  body  the  same  relation  as  does  a 


X]        SEMIDECUSSATION    OF   OPTIC  TRACTS        385 

limb  on  one  side  of  the  body.  Thus,  the  external  rectus  muscle 
of  the  right  eyeball  bears  the  same  lateral  relation  to  the 
median  sagittal  plane  of  the  body  as  does  the  right  arm.  And 
the  internal  rectus  muscle  of  the  left  eyeball  bears  the  same 
lateral  relation  to  the  median  sagittal  plane  of  the  body  as  does 
the  right  arm.  Now  a  general  arrangement  evident  in  the 
cerebral  cortex  is  that  the  taxis  of  muscles  lying  to  right  of 
the  median  sagittal  plane  is  entrusted  to  the  left  hemisphere, 
and  vice  versa.  It  is  in  accord  with  this  that  in  animals  with 
overlapping  visual  fields  the  horizontal  movement  of  the  eyes 
to  one  side  should  for  both  eyeballs  be  represented  in  07ie  and 
the  same  hemisphere.  And  as  a  fact  the  conjugate  movement 
is  found  represented  for  both  eyeballs  together  in  each  hemis- 
phere. If  we  regard,  and  it  was  shown  above  that  we  may  do 
so,  the  median  sagittal  planes  of  both  eyeballs  as  identical  with 
the  median  sagittal  plane  of  the  head,  they  are  identical  with 
each  other,  and  the  scheme  of  cortical  representation  may  be 
expressed  thus :  a  point  in  the  right  retina  and  its  twin  point  in 
the  left  demand  each  of  them  identical  movement  of  the  two 
eyeballs  when,  apart  from  convergence,  those  points  excite  their 
own  replacement  by  the  fovea  {i.  e,  when  initiating  a  gaze).  It 
is  obvious  that  the  paths  from  the  visual  cortex  of  each  side  to 
the  eyeball  muscles  —  experiment  shows  such  a  path  to  exist  — 
is  connected  therefore  with  both  the  right  and  left  twin  points. 
That  is,  it  is  a  common  path.  The  confluence  of  conductors  from 
the  two  retinae  to  the  same  cortical  field,  though  not  uniting  their 
retinal  impressions,  gives  them  access  to  a  common  efferent  path 
which  both  must  use.*  At  entrance  to  every  common  path  Hes, 
as  shown  before,  a  co-ordinating  mechanism.  A  co-ordinative 
mechanism  is  thus  obtained.  This  "  common  path "  with  its 
bilateral  twin  origin  impinges  in  its  turn,  directly  or  indirectly,  on 
the  motor  neurones  for  the  lateral  eye-muscles,  ^^  final  common 
paths.  We  have  therefore  to  alter  such  a  scheme  as  that  furnished 
by  Cajal  by  attaching  his  convergent  paths  to  efferent  paths,  and 

*  Mott  has  likewise  independently  urged  that  the  interpretation  to  be  placed  on 
this  convergence  of  paths  is  motor  rather  than  sensory.  Trans.  Ophthalm.  Soc,  vol. 
25,  p.  cii,  1905. 

25 


386  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

by  divesting  their  supposed  nodal  cortical  point  of  its  hypo- 
thetical powers  as  a  sensual  Deus  ex  machina.  And  we  thus 
meet  another  instance  of  convergence  of  afferent  paths  leading 
to  motor  synthesis,  but  not,  or  only  remotely,  to  sensual.  Seen 
in  this  light  the  gulf  between  sensation  and  movement  looms 
even  wider  than  was  allowed  for  in  the  tentative  suppositions 
which  prompted  the  above  experiments  on  flicker. 

We  are  thus  warned  against  any  hasty  conclusion  that  the 
neural  mechanisms  which  synthesize  reflex  movements  illustrate 
in  their  arrangement  also  those  concerned  where  sensual  fusion 
is  the  phenomenon.  But  that  does  not  invalidate  a  broad  prac- 
tical inference  which  study  of  the  nervous  system  in  regard  to 
motor  reaction  allows.  This  inference  is  that  toward  the  solu- 
tion of  the  problems  of  motor  taxis  help  is  obtainable  by  appeal 
to  characters  evident  in  sensual  reaction.  This  practical  infer- 
ence need  not  in  the  least  involve  any  doctrinal  attitude  what- 
ever toward  the  hypothesis  of  psycho-physical  parallelism.  It 
may  proceed  quite  apart  from  that.  It  simply  insists  on  the 
likeness  of  nervous  reactions  expressed  by  muscular  and  other 
effector-organs  to  reactions  whose  evidence  is  sensual.  It  in- 
sists on  this  likeness  being  close  and  fundamental  enough  to 
make  each  of  the  two  classes  of  phenomena  of  use  to  the 
student  of  the  other.  A  number  of  excellent  investigators 
hold,  on  the  opposite  hand,  that  the  study  of  the  two  should 
proceed  apart  even  more  rigidly  than  they  do  at  present.  Con- 
fusion has,  it  is  true,  been  caused  in  both  by  the  loose  applica- 
tion of  the  terms  of  the  one  set  of  phenomena  to  the  other.  But 
to  disregard  the  many  significant  similarities  which  exist  between 
the  two  sets  is,  it  seems  to  me,  to  throw  away  one  of  the  best 
instruments  for  discovery  in  both.  We  saw  how  suggestive 
psychological  data  prove  for  classifying  the  various  species 
of  receptors  considered  as  initiators  of  reflexes.  The  after- 
discharge  of  a  nervous  arc  finds  expression  not  only  in  reflex 
movement  but  in,  for  instance,  a  visual  after-image.  Centripetal 
impulses  from  eye-muscles  reinforce  visual  {i.  e.  extero-ceptor) 
sensations  (Macdougall)  just  as  centripetal  impulses  from  the 
leg  muscles  reinforce  reflex  movement  induced  from  the  skin 


X]  DISTINCT   FROM   REFLEX   UNION  387 

(extero-ceptor)  of  the  foot.  The  "  immediate  spinal  induction" 
exemphfied  by  reflexes  has  a  counterpart  in  visual  irradiation. 
Visual  contrast,  if  translated  into  terms  of  reflex  contraction, 
bears  close  resemblance  to  "  successive  spinal  induction."  The 
features  of  fatigue  repeat  themselves  in  both  sets  of  phenomena. 
Receptors  which  initiate  reflex  movements  adapted  in  regard 
to  objects  at  a  distance  initiate  as  sense-organs  sensations  pro- 
jected into  circumambient  sensual  space.  Receptors  which 
initiate  reflex  movements  advantageous  in  regard  to  some 
locus  of  the  surface  of  the  body  itself,  e,  g,  removal  of  irrita- 
tion thence,  initiate  as  sense-organs  sensations  referred  to  that 
same  locus.  Instances  might  be  multiplied,  but  they  have  risen 
prominently  in  several  of  the  foregoing  lectures,  and  are  suffi- 
ciently before  our  minds  now.  A  practical  inference  from  them 
is  that  physiology  and  psychology,  instead  of  prosecuting  their 
studies,  as  some  now  recommend,  more  strictly  apart  one  from 
another  than  at  present,  will  find  it  serviceable  for  each  to  give 
to  the  results  achieved  by  the  other  even  closer  heed  than  has 
been  customary  hitherto. 

Besides  this  similarity  of  time-relation  and  other  features 
between  the  physiological  and  the  psychical  signs  of  neural 
activity,  another  link  connects  the  psychological  and  the  physi- 
ological for  the  biologist.  To  the  physiology  of  pure  reflexes, 
that  is,  reflexes  devoid  of  psychical  accompaniment  so  far 
as  introspection  can  discover,  psychological  interest  neverthe- 
less attaches,  and  on  a  very  distinct  ground.  This  ground  of 
connection  is  seen  if  inquiry  is  followed  along  the  animal  scale 
in  the  direction  from  higher  forms  to  lower  rather  than  by  the 
usually  more  favorable  reverse  approach.  This  is  partly  because 
we  directly  observe  psychical  phenonema  by  introspection  only, 
that  is,  only  in  ourselves ;  and  the  facts  discovered  by  introspec- 
tion are  applicable  to  other  beings  the  more  readily  the  more 
those  beings  resemble  ourselves,  namely,  are  animals  ranking 
near  to  man. 

Pure  reflexes  are  admirably  adapted  to  certain  ends.  They 
are  reactions  which  have  long  proved  advantageous  in  the  phy- 
lum, of  which  the  existent  individual  is  a  representative  embodi- 


388  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

ment.  Perfected  during  the  course  of  ages,  they  have  during 
that  course  attained  a  stability,  a  certainty,  and  an  ease  of  per- 
formance beside  which  the  stability  and  facility  of  the  most 
ingrained  habit  acquired  during  an  individual  life  is  presumably 
small.  But  theirs  is  of  itself  a  machine-like  fatality.  Their 
character  in  this  stands  revealed  when  the  neural  arcs  which 
execute  them  are  separated,  e.  g.  by  transection  of  the  spinal 
cord,  from  the  higher  centres  of  the  nervous  system.  They  can 
be  checked,  it  is  true,  as  we  have  seen,  by  coUision  with  other 
reflexes  as  ancestral  and  as  fatally  operative  as  themselves 
(Lectures  V  and  VI).  To  these  ancient  invariable  reflexes,  con- 
sciousness, in  the  ordinary  meaning  of  the  term,  is  not  adjunct. 
The  subject  as  active  agent  does  not  direct  them  and  cannot 
introspect  them. 

Yet  it  is  clear,  in  higher  animals  especially  so,  that  reflexes 
are  under  control.  Their  intrinsic  fatality  lies  under  control  by 
higher  centres  unless  their  nervous  arcs  are  sundered  from  ties 
existing  with  those  higher  centres.  In  other  words,  the  re- 
actions of  reflex-arcs  are  controllable  by  mechanisms  to  whose 
activity  consciousness  is  adjunct.  By  these  higher  centres,  this 
or  that  reflex  can  be  checked,  or  released,  or  modified  in  its  re- 
action with  such  variety  and  seeming  independence  of  external 
stimuli  that  the  existence  of  a  spontaneous  internal  process  ex- 
pressed as  "  will "  is  the  naive  inference  drawn.  Its  spring  of 
action  is  not  now  our  question ;  its  seat  in  the  nervous  system 
seems  to  correspond  with  that  of  processes  of  perceptual  level. 
It  is  urgently  necessary  for  physiology  to  know  how  this  con- 
trol—  volitional  control  —  is  operative  upon  reflexes,  that  is, 
how  it  intrudes  and  makes  its  influence  felt  upon  the  running 
of  the  reflex  machinery.  How  is  the  cough,  or  eye-closure,  or 
the  impulse  to  smile  suppressed?  How  is  the  convergence  of 
the  eyeballs,  innately  associate  to  visual  fixation  of  a  near  ob- 
ject, initiated  voluntarily  without  recourse  to  fixation  on  an 
object?  Or  how  is  the  innate  respiratory  rhythm  voluntarily 
modified  to  meet  the  passing  requirements  of  vocal  utterance? 
No  exposition  of  the  integrative  action  of  the  nervous  system  is 
complete,  even  in  outline,  if  this  control  is  left  without  considera- 


X]  REFLEX  AND   VOLITIONAL  ACTION  389 

tion.  Reflexes  ordinarily  outside  its  pale  can  by  training  be 
brought  within  it.  The  actor,  it  is  asserted,  can  shed  tears  at 
will,  or  blush  or  blanch.  Occasional  instances  are  recorded  of 
power  to  slow  the  rhythm  of  the  heart  at  will ;  others,  of  power 
to  suppress  the  reflex  of  swallowing  when  it  has  entered  on  its 
pharyngeal  stage.  Volitional  movement  can  certainly  become 
involuntary,  and,  conversely,  involuntary  movements  can  some- 
times be  brought  under  subjection  to  the  will.  From  this  subjec- 
tion it  is  but  a  short  step  to  acquisition  of  coordinations  which 
express  themselves  as  movements  newly  acquired  by  the  indi- 
vidual. The  controlling  centres  can  pick  out  from  an  ancestrally 
given  motor  reaction  some  one  part  of  it,  so  as  to  isolate  that  as 
a  new  separate  movement,  and  by  enhancement  this  can  become 
a  skilled  adapted  act  added  to  the  powers  of  the  individual.  In 
regard  to  the  ring  finger,  the  motor  co-ordination  ancestrally  pro- 
vided gives  extension  of  that  finger  only  in  company  with  the 
fingers  on  each  side  of  it.  We  can  soon  train  ourselves  to  lift 
the  ring-finger  alone  without  the  others.  The  "will "  dissociates 
the  ancestral  co-ordination.  Similarly  we  can  acquire  the  power 
to  move  a  part  which  neither  reflexly  nor  otherwise  would  seem 
to  come  within  the  scope  of  our  voluntary  innervation,  although 
of  course  there  must  be  motor  nerve  and  muscle  upon  which 
our  innervation  can  operate.  Thus,  we  can  learn  to  retract 
the  pinna  of  the  ear ;  the  movement  is  at  first  accompanied  by 
other  facial  movement,  but  later  with  practice  it  becomes  exe- 
cutable without  other  facial  movement.  A  new  reaction  and 
co-ordination  has  been  gained  by  the  individual. 

The  transition  from  reflex  action  to  volitional  is  not  abrupt 
and  sharp.  Familiar  instances  of  individual  acquisition  of  motor 
co-ordination  are  furnished  by  the  cases  in  which  short,  simple 
movements,  whether  reflex  or  not,  are  by  practice  under  voli- 
tion combined  into  new  sequences  and  become  in  time  habitual 
in  the  sense  that  though  able  to  be  directed  they  no  longer  re- 
quire concentration  of  attention  upon  them  for  their  execution. 
As  I  write,  my  mind  is  not  preoccupied  with  how  my  fingers  form 
the  letters;  my  attention  is  fixed  simply  on  the  thought  the 
words  express.     But  there  was  a  time  when  the  formation  of  the 


390  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

letters,  as  each  one  was  written,  would  have  occupied  my  whole 
attention. 

Volitional  control  of  reflexes  is  a  question  of  co-ordina- 
tion not  explicitly  before  us  previously  in  these  lectures.  Its 
analysis  has  not  indeed  proceeded  far.  We  may  premise  that 
some  extension  of  the  same  processes  outlined  in  Lectures  V 
and  VI,  as  operative  in  simultaneous  combination  and  in  suc- 
cessive combination  of  reflexes,  must  be  operative  in  this  con- 
trol. There  we  saw  reflexes  modifying  each  other,  and  the  more 
complex  reactions  being  built  up  from  simpler  and  more  re-* 
stricted  ones.  Some  extension  of  the  same  process  should,  in 
view  of  our  inferences  regarding  the  nature  of  the  dominance  of 
the  brain  (Lecture  IX),  apply  here  also. 

It  is  significant  that,  although  the  reflexes  controlled  are 
so  often  unconscious,  consciousness  is  adjunct  to  the  centres 
which  exert  the  control.  A  biologist.  Professor  Lloyd  Morgan, 
has  urged  that  *'the  primary  aim,  object,  and  purpose  of  con- 
sciousness is  control.  Consciousness  in  a  mere  automaton  is  a 
useless  and  unnecessary  epiphenomenon."  *  A  somewhat  similar 
thought  rose  incidentally  to  our  lips  in  a  previous  lecture  (Lec- 
ture IX).  The  pleasure-pain  accompaniment  of  reflexes  has  often 
been  interpreted  as  carrying  that  meaning.  Certain  it  is  that  if  we 
study  the  process  by  which  in  ourselves  this  control  over  reflex 
action  is  acquired  by  an  individual,  psychical  factors  loom  large, 
and  more  is  known  of  them  than  of  the  purely  physiological  modus 
operandi  involved  in  the  attainment  of  the  control.  Hence, 
psychological  studies  have  been  more  numerous  than  physio- 
logical in  this  field.  It  is  found  that  kinaesthetic  sensations  of 
the  movement  to  be  acquired  or  controlled,  though  helpful,  are 
less  important  than  the  resident  sensations  from  the  part  in  its 
"  resting  "  state.  These  latter,  with  the  power  to  focus  atten- 
tion upon  them,  appear,  in  a  number  of  instances,  to  be  a  most 
necessary  condition  for  the  acquirement  of  the  control.  And 
in  the  monkey,  voluntary  control  of  a  limb  is  largely  lost  when 
the  limb  has  been  rendered  apaesthetic.^^^ 

A  biological  inference  arises  at  this  point.     We  have  admitted 

*  Introduction  to  Comparative  Psychology,  London,  1894,  p.  182. 


X]      NERVOUS  ORGANS  OF  CONTROL      391 

that  the  organs  to  which  psychosis  is  adjunct,  namely,  the  brain, 
and  especially  in  higher  vertebrates  the  cerebral  hemispheres, 
supply  the  surest  touchstone  to  rank  in  the  scale  of  animal 
creation.  That  is  to  admit,  in  other  words,  that  development 
of  these  organs  constitutes,  on  the  whole,  the  best  criterion  to 
the  success  of  an  animal  form  in  the  competition  which  lies  at 
the  root  of  animal  evolution.  These  organs,  we  have  just 
seen,  are  the  organs  of  nervous  control ;  and  that  control  is  ex- 
ercised mainly  in  the  perfecting  and  readjusting  of  manoeuvres 
of  ancient  heritage.  The  way  in  which  we  ourselves  acquire  a 
new  skilled  movement,  the  means  by  which  we  get  more  pre- 
cission  and  speed  in  the  use  of  a  tool,  the  handling  of  an  instru- 
ment, or  marksmanship  with  a  weapon,  is  by  a  process  of  learning 
in  which  nervous  organs  of  control  modify  the  activities  of  reflex 
centres,  themselves  already  perfected  for  other  though  kindred 
actions.  Our  process  of  learning  is  accompanied  by  conscious 
effort  These  nervous  organs  of  control  form,  therefore,  a  special 
instrument  of  adaptation  and  of  readjustment  of  reaction  to 
better  suit  requirements  which  may  be  new.  New  adaptations 
whence  the  individual  may  reap  benefit  are  thus  attained.  The 
more  complex  an  organism,  the  more  points  of  contact  it  has 
with  the  environment,  and  the  more  frequently  will  it  need 
readjustment  amid  an  environment  of  shifting  relationships. 
These  nervous  organs  of  control  being  organs  of  adjustment  will 
be  more  prominent  the  further  the  animal  scale  is  followed  up- 
ward to  its  crowning  species,  man.  And  these  organs  which 
give  adjustability  to  the  running  of  the  reflex  machinery,  as 
such,  seem  themselves  —  perhaps,  by  reason  of  their  constant 
relative  newness  —  to  be  among  the  most  plastic  in  the  body. 
In  man  and  the  species  near  him,  these  organs  are  most  de- 
veloped, and  their  mechanisms  are  cerebral.  These  cerebral 
mechanisms  constitute  the  clearest  criterion  of  evolutional  suc- 
cess. In  these  types  it  is  cerebral  function  which  best  compasses 
that  modification  of  old  and  that  development  of  new  reaction, 
which  perfects  the  adaptation  of  the  individual  to  the  environ- 
ment. The  relatively  high  development  in  man  of  this  organ  for 
individual  adjustment  of  reactions  makes  him  the  most  successful 


392  SENSUAL  FUSION  [Lect. 

animal  on  earth's  surface  at  the  present  epoch.  No  doubt  the 
greater  part  of  all  this  adjustment  of  reaction  will,  in  his  case,  as 
he  stands  now,  come  under  intellectual  activity.  In  him  reason 
enables  the  individual  profitably  to  forecast  the  future,  and  to 
act  the  more  suitably  to  meet  it,  from  memory  of  the  past.  Mere 
experience  can,  however,  apart  from  reason,  mould  nervous  reac- 
tions in  so  far  as  they  are  plastic.  The  *'  bahmmg'*  of  a  reflex 
exhibits  this  faculty  in  germ.  In  the  humble  spheres  of  nervous 
activity,  such  as  alone  fall  within  the  scope  of  these  lectures, 
simple  sensori-motor  experience  seems  to  count  for  more  than 
reason  in  the  actual  process  of  acquiring  new  motor  co-ordina- 
tions. Of  course  reason,  directing  effort,  counts  in  the  selection 
of  the  field  of  operation  of  motor  experience.  But  the  inefficacy, 
as  a  means  to  arrive  at  a  new  motor  correlation,  of  instruction 
merely  verbal,  or  of  ideas  constructed  without  motor  experience, 
is  common  knowledge.  To  learn  skating  or  racquets  by  simple 
cogitation  or  visual  observation  is,  of  course,  impossible.  Here 
mere  sensori-motor  experience  is  more  valuable  than  any  course 
of  reasoning  can  be.  Hence  the  training  for  a  new  skilled 
motor  manoeuvre  must  be  simply  ad  hoCy  and  is  of  itself  no 
training  for  another  motor  co-ordination,  —  apart  from  the  well- 
known  mutual  influence  of  training  on  symmetrical  parts  of  the 
body.  Yet,  in  high  animal  types,  the  connection  between  skilled 
movements  and  the  so-called  **  motor  *'  region  of  the  cortex 
cerebri,  and  the  defect  in  these  which  injury  of  that  region  en- 
tails, countenances  the  belief  that  the  "  experience  "  involved  in 
this  training,  though  not  rational,  is  cerebral.  The  compensation 
of  co-ordi native  defects  which  the  cerebrum  accomplishes  after 
cerebellar  or  labyrinthine  lesions,  points  to  a  similar  conclusion. 
And  we  must  remember  that  though  the  mere  sensori-motor 
experience  counts  for  so  much  in  the  mastering  of  a  new  move- 
ment, they  are  perceptual,  and  in  man  rational,  processes  which 
initiate,  maintain,  and  guide  effort  toward  acquirement  of  an  act 
which  is  new. 

We  thus,  from  the  biological  standpoint,  see  the  cerebrum, 
and  especially  the  cerebral  cortex,  as  the  latest  and  highest  ex- 
pression of  a  nervous  mechanism  which  may  be  described  as  the 


X]  THE   CEREBRUM  393 

organ  of^  mid  for ^  the  adaptation  of  nervous  reactions.  The  cere- 
brum, built  upon  the  distance-receptors  and  entrusted  with  reac- 
tions which  fall  in  an  anticipatory  interval  so  as  to  be  precurrent 
(Lect.  IX),  comes,  with  its  projici^nce  of  sensation  and  the 
psychical  powers  unfolded  from  that  germ  of  advantage,  to  be 
the  orgdin  par  excellence  for  the  readjustment  and  the  perfecting 
of  the  nervous  reactions  of  the  animal  as  a  whole,  so  as  to  im- 
prove and  extend  their  suitability  to,  and  advantage  over,  the 
environment.  These  adjustments,  though  not  transmitted  to 
the  offspring,  yet  in  higher  animals  form  the  most  potent  in- 
ternal condition  for  enabling  the  species  to  maintain  and  increase 
in  sum  its  dominance  over  the  environment  in  which  it  is  im- 
mersed. A  certain  measure  of  such  dominance  is  its  ancestral 
heritage ;  on  this  is  based  its  innate  right  to  success  in  the  com- 
petition for  existence.  But  the  factors  and  elements  of  that 
competition  change  in  detail  as  the  history  of  the  earth  pro- 
ceeds. The  creature  has  to  be  partially  readjusted  if  it  is  to 
hold  its  own  in  the  struggle.  Only  by  continual  modification 
of  its  ancestral  powers  to  suit  the  present  can  it  fulfil  that  which 
its  destiny,  if  it  is  to  succeed,  requires  from  it  as  its  life's  purpose, 
namely,  the  extension  of  its  dominance  over  its  environment. 
For  this  conquest  its  cerebrum  is  its  best  weapon.  It  is  then 
around  the  cerebrum,  its  physiological  and  psychological  attri- 
butes, that  the  main  interest  of  biology  must  ultimately  turn. 


BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 


I. 

1648. 

2. 

1662. 

3- 

1671. 

4- 

1677. 

5- 

1704. 

6. 

1752. 

6a. 

1768. 

7- 

1775- 

8. 

1775- 

9- 

1805. 

10. 

1811. 

II. 

1822. 

12. 

1823. 

1 2a. 

1824. 

1 2b 

1825. 

13- 

1829. 

14. 

1834. 

IS- 

1835- 

16. 

1837. 

17. 

1838. 

18. 

1842. 

19. 

1842. 

20. 

1843. 

21. 

1846. 

22. 

1847. 

23- 

1850. 

24. 

1853. 

25- 

1858. 

26. 

i860. 

27. 

1861. 

28. 

1861. 

29. 

1 861. 

30- 

1862. 

31- 

1863. 

32- 

1863. 

33- 

1864. 

Descartes,  R.     Passions  de  Tame.     Paris. 

Descartes,  R.     De  Homine.     Edit,  by  Schuyl.     Leyden. 

Rohault.     Traite  de  physique,  Part  I.     Paris. 

Descartes,  R.     De  Homine.     Edit,  by  de  la  Forge.     Amsterdam. 

Newton,  I.     Opticks.     Prop.  112. 

Haller,  A.  von.      De  partibus  corporis  humani  sentientibus  et  non- 

sentientibus.     Gottingen. 
Spallanzani.     Sopra  la  reproduzione.     Modena. 
Smith-KSstner.     Lehrbegriff  d.  Optik. 
Harris.     Opticks,     London. 

Bichat.     Rech.  physiol.  sur  la  vie  et  la  mort,  iii.     Paris. 
Bell,  C.     A  New  Idea  of  the  Anat.  of  the  Brain.    London. 
Majendie.     Journ.  de  physiol.  experimentale. 
Bell,  C.     Phil.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London. 
Wollaston.     Phil.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London. 
Herbart.     Psychologie  als  Wissenschaft,  Konigsberg. 
Bell,  C.     Anat.  and  Physiol,  of  the  Human  Body,  by  J.  and  C.  Bell. 

7th  edition,  corrected  by  C.  Bell.    The  passage  referred  to  appears 

as  a  footnote,  p.  110. 
Talbot.     Phil.  Mag.,  3.  5.,  p.  327. 
Plateau,  J.     Poggend.  Annal.,   xxxv,  p.  457. 
Grainger.     On  the  Structure  of  the  Spinal  Cord.     London. 
Volkmann.     Miiller's  Archiv,  p.  87. 
Volkmann.     Miiller's  Archiv,  p.  372. 
Flourens.     Rech.  exper.  s.  1.  fonct.  d.  syst.  nerv.     Paris. 
Mtiller,  J.     Elem.  of  Physiol.,  transl.  by  Baly.     London. 
Weber,  E.  H.  and  Ed.     Omodeiannali  univers.  di  med.,  cxvi,  p.  225. 
Traube,  J.     Med.  Zeitung  d.  Vereins  f.  Heilk.,  v. 
Marshall  Hall.    Synopsis  of  the  Diastaltic  Nervous  System.    London. 
Pflueger,  Ed.     Die  sensor.  Funkt.  d.  Riickenm.     Berlin. 
Harless.     Abhandl.  d.  Baeyr.  Akad.  d.  Physik.,  xxxi. 
Fechner.     Abh.  d.  Akad.  Wiss.  Leipzig,  vii,  p.  423. 
Rosenthal,  J.     Compt.  rend.,  i,  p.  754. 
Rosenthal,  J.     Unters.  z.  Naturlehre,  viii,  p.  312. 
Broca,  P.     Bull.  d.  1.  Soc.  Anat.  d.  Paris. 
Rosenthal,  J.      Die    Atembeweg.   u.  ihre   Bezieh.  z.  nerv.   Vagus. 

Berlin. 
Setschenow,  J.     Physiol.  Studien  ii.  d.  Hemmungsmech.  f.  d.  Re- 

flexthat.  d.  Riickenm.  u.  Gehirn  d.  Frosch.     Berlin. 
Setschenow,  J.     Ann.  d.  Sci.  Natur.,  xix,  p.  109. 
Setschenow,  J.    Zeitschr.  f.  rat.  Med.,  xxiii,  Nr.  6. 


396  BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 

Hering,  E.     Beitr.  z.  Physiol.,  Heft,  v,  p.  310,     Leipzig. 

Hughlings-Jackson.     Lond.  Hosp.  Rep.,  i,  p.  459. 

Cayrade.     Les  mouvements  reflexes.     Paris. 

Aubert.     Physiol,  d.  Netzhaut,  p.  287.     Breslau. 

Setschenow,  J.     Zeitschr.  f.  rat.  med.,  xxvi,  p.  292. 

Cyon,  V.     Ber.  d.  k.  Sachs.  Gesellsch.  d.  Wiss.  z.  Leipzig. 

Goltz,  F.     Centralb.  f.  d.  med.  Wiss.,  p.  705. 

Cyon,  von.     Ludwig's  Arbeiten.     Leipzig. 

Von  Bezold  u.  Uspensky.     Centralb.  f.  d.  med.  Wiss.,  p.  611. 

Hering,    E.      Die    Selbststeuerung    d.    Athmung   durch  d.   Nervus 
Vagus,  Wien  Akad.,  Ivii,  2. 

Breuer,  J.     Wien.  Akad.,  Iviii,  2. 

Goltz,  F.     Beitrage  z.  Lehre  v.  d.  Funkt.  d.  Nervencentra  d.  Frosches. 
Berlin. 

Bastian,  C.     Journ.  of  Ment.  Sciences.     London. 

Fritsch  and  Hitzig,  E.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  300. 

Nothnagel.     Virchow's  Archiv,  xlix,  p.  267. 

Hering,  E.     Sitzb.  d.  Wien.  Akad.,  Ixvi-lxx,  Abt.  iii. 

Duchenne,  G.  B.  A.     L'electrisation  localisee.     Paris. 

Darwin,  C.     Expressions  of  the  Emotions.     London. 

Ferricr,  D.     West  Riding  Lunatic  Asyl.  Rep.     London. 

Valerius.     Poggendorff's  Annal.,  cl,  p.  17. 

Hitzig,  E.     Untersuch.  iib.  d.  Gehirn.     Berlin. 

Goltz  and  Freusberg.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  viii,  p.  460. 

Kronecker,  H.  (with  W.  Stirling).     Festsch.  z.  Ludwig.    Leipzig. 

V.  Cyon.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  viii,  p.  347. 

Stirling,  W.     Ludwig's  Arbeiten,  p.  245.     Leipzig. 

Goltz,  F.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  ix,  p.  552. 

Exner,  S.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  viii. 

Freusberg,  A.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  ix,  p.  358. 

Freusberg,  A.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  x,  p.  174. 

Freusberg,  A.     Archiv  f.  expt.  Path.  u.  Pharm.,  iii,  p.  17 

Darwin,  C.     Insectivorous  Plants.     London. 

Erb,  W.     Berl.  Klin.  Wochenschr.,  Nr.  26. 

Erb,  W.     Archiv  f.  Psychiat.  u.  Nervenkr.,  v,  p.  792. 

Westphal,  C.     Archiv  f.  Psychiat.  u.  Nervenkr.,  v,  p.  803. 

Aubert.     Physiologische  Optik,  p.  500.     Leipzig. 

Mosso,  A.     Molesch.  Untersuch.,  ii,  p.  331. 

Marey,  E.  J.     Trav.  d.  Laboratoire,  ii.     Paris. 

Gergens.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xiii,  p.  68. 

Wundt,  W.     Ueb.  d.  Reflex  vorgang  u.  d.  Wesen,  d.  central.  Inner- 
vation.    Stuttgart. 

Ferrier,  D.     Functions  of  the  Brain.     London. 

Flechsig,  P.   Die  Leitungsbahnen  im  Gehirn  u.  Riickenmark.  Leipzig. 

Exner,  S.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  567. 

Romanes,  J.     Philos.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  clvii. 

Babuchin.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  66.     Leipzig. 

Langendorff,  O.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  96. 

Langendorff,  O.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  435. 

Hughlings-Jackson.     The  Med.  Examiner.     London. 


34. 

1864. 

35- 

1864. 

36. 

1864. 

37. 

1865. 

38. 

1865. 

39- 

1865. 

40. 

1865. 

41. 

1866. 

42. 

1867. 

43- 

1868. 

44. 

1868. 

45- 

1869. 

46. 

1869. 

47. 

1870. 

48. 

1870. 

48a. 

1872-4. 

49. 

1872. 

50. 

1872. 

51- 

1873- 

52- 

1873- 

S3- 

1874. 

54. 

1874. 

55- 

1874. 

56. 

1874. 

57- 

1874. 

58. 

1874. 

59- 

1874- 

59a. 

1874. 

60. 

1875. 

61. 

1875- 

62. 

1875. 

63- 

1875. 

64. 

1875. 

65. 

1875. 

65a. 

1876. 

66. 

1876. 

67. 

1876. 

68. 

1876. 

69. 

1876. 

70. 

1876. 

70a 

1876. 

71- 

1877. 

72. 

1877. 

73- 

1877. 

74- 

1877. 

74a. 

1877. 

75- 

1877. 

BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 


397 


[878. 
[878. 
[878. 

[879. 
[879. 

1879. 
[879. 
[880. 
[880. 
[880. 
[880. 
[880. 

;i. 

ii. 
[881. 
[881. 

[881. 

:88i. 

;i. 

[881. 

;i. 
[881. 

t88i. 
[882. 
1882. 
[882. 
1883. 


[884. 
[884. 
[884. 
[885. 
[885. 
:88s. 
[886. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
[887. 
:887. 
[888. 


Tschiriew,  S.     Archiv  f.  Psychiat  u.  Nervenkr.,  viii,  p.  689. 

Griitzner,  P.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xvii,  p.  238. 

Hughlings-Jackson.     The  Med.  Examiner.     London. 

Martin,  Newell,  and  Hartwell,  E.  M.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  ii. 

Ward,  J,     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  ii. 

Ott,  J.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  ii,  p.  48. 

Spode,  O.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  115. 

James,  W.     Bost.  Soc.  of  Nat.  Hist. 

Wundt,  W.     Grundziige  der  Physiolog.  Psychol. 

Swinton,  A.  H.     Insect  Variety.     London. 

Spencer,  H.     Principles  of  Psychology.     London. 

Schloesser,  E.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.     Leipzig. 

Goltz,  F.     Verricht.  d.  Grosshirns.     Strassburg. 

Ott,  J.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  iii. 

Kronecker,  H.,  and  Meltzer,  S.  J.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  465. 

Kronecker,  H.,  and  Meltzer,  S.  J.     Monatsb.  d.  Berlin.  Akad.  d. 

Wiss.,  Feb.  24. 
Walton.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  iii,  p.  308. 

Bubnoff,  N.,  and  Heidenhain,  R.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xxvi,  p.  137. 
Munk,  H.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  553. 
Heidenhain,  R.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xxvi,  p.  546. 
Gad,  J.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  566. 
Bert,  P.,  and  Marcacci.     Lo  sperimentali,  Nr.  10. 
Ferrier,  D.,  and  Yeo,  G.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Lond.,  xxxii. 
Richet,  C.     Physiol,  d.  Muscles  et  d.  Nervs.     Paris. 
Exner,  S.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xxviii,  p.  487. 
Gaskell,  W.  H.     Philosoph.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc,  London,  p.  999. 
Meltzer,  S.  J.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Bd.,  p.  209. 
Kronecker,  H.,  and  Meltzer,  S.  J.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Ed., 

p.  328. 
Gad,  J.     66te.  Versamml.  d.  deutsch.  Naturf.  u.  Aertze. 
Gaskell,  W.  H.     Trans.  8th  Intemat.  Med.  Congress.     Copenhagen. 
Schreiber,  J.     Deutsch.  Archiv  f.  klin.  Med.,  xxxv,  p.  254. 
Jendr^ssik,  E.     Neurol.  Centralb.,  p.  412. 
Beaunis,  H.     Compt.  rend.,  c,  p.  918. 
Lange.     Om  Sindsbevagelser.     Copenhagen. 
Mitchell  and  Lewis.     Med.  News,  Feb.  13.     Philadelphia. 
Brunton,  Lauder  T.     Text-book  of  Pharmacology,  ed.  3,  p.  168. 
Gad,  J.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.     Verhandl.  d.  phys.  Ges.,  June,  p.  468. 
Biedermann,  W.     Sitzb.  d.  Akad.  d.  Wiss.  Wien,  xciii,  3,  p.  8. 
Exner,  S.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  567. 
Franck,  Fr.     Le9ons  sur  les  fonct.  d.  cerveau.     Paris. 
Schafer,  E.  A.     Festschrift  zu  Karl  Ludwig.     Leipzig. 
Nansen,  F.     Bergen's  Museum. 
Albertoni.     Arch,  italiennes  de  Biolog.,  Part  I. 
Tarchanow.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xl,  p.  340. 

Hering,  E.     Z.  Theor.  d.  Vorgange  i.  d.  lebend.  Substanz.    Prag. 
James,  W.     Scribner's  Mag.     New  York. 
Gad,  J.     Eulenberg's  Real-encycl.,  xvi,  p.  673. 
Lombard,  W.  P.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Psychol.,  p.  8. 


398  BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 

Bowditch,  H.  P.     Boston  Med.  and  Surg.  Journ.,  Nr.  32. 

Schafer,  E.  A.,  and  Brown,  Sanger.     Philos.  Trans,  R.  S.  London. 

Head,  H.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  x,  p.  i. 

Lombard,  W.  P.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Bd.,  p.  292. 

Wernicke.     Berliner  Klin.  Wochenschr. 

Beaunis,  H.     Arch,  de  physiol.  norm,  et  path.,  p.  55. 

Gad,  J.,  and  Joseph,  M.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  p.  199. 

Mott,  F.  W.,  and  Schafer,  E.  A.     Brain,  xiii.     London. 

Belmondo  and  Oddi.     Lab.  di  Fisiologia.     Florence. 

Bowditch,  H.  P.,  and  Warren,  J.  W.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xi,  p.  25. 

James,  W.     Text-book  of  Psychology.     London. 

Waller,  A.  D.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xi,  p.  384. 

Haycraft,  J.     Brain,  xii,  p.  516. 

Beevor,  C,  and  Horsley,  V.     Phil.  Trans.  R.  S.,  B.,  p.  128. 

Munk,  H.,  and  Obregia.     Sitz.  d.  Berlin.  Akad.  d.  Wiss.,  Jan.  23. 

Ramon-y-Cajal.    Gazett.  med.  Catalan. 

Bastian,  C.     Medico-chirurg.  Soc.  Trans.     London. 

Sternberg,  M.     Sitz.  d.  K.  K.  Akad.  d.  W.,  Wien,  c,  3,  288. 

Bowlby,  A.     Medico-chirurg.  Soc.  Trans.     London. 

Waller,  A.  D.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xii. 

Gotch,  F.,  and  Horsley,  V.    Croonian  Lecture,  R.  S.  London. 

Polimanti,  O.     Archives  italiennes  d.  Biol.,  xxiii. 

Munk,  H.     Sitz.  d.  k.  Preus.  Akad.  d.  Wiss.  xxxvi,  p.  691. 

Beevor,  C.     Brain,  Part  53,  p.  51. 

Goltz,  F.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  li. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xiii,  p.  621. 

Ewald,  J.  R.     Endorgan  d.  Nervus  Octavus.     Wiesbaden. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Physiol.  Soc,  Feb.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xiii. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Philos.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  Lond.,  clxxxiv,  B. 

Bruns.     Neurol.  Centralb. 

Piotrowski,  G.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xiv,  p.  163. 

Sternberg,  M.     Die  Sehnenreflexe.  Wien. 

Head,  H.     Brain,  Parts  61,  62. 

Bayliss,  W.  M.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xiv,  p.  303. 

Ferrier,  D.     Brain,  Spring  Number. 

Cybulski  and  Zanietowski.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ivi. 

Nagel,  W.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ivii,  p.  495. 

Langendorff  and  Oldag.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  lix,  p.  206. 

V.  Frey.     Ber.  d.  k.  sachs.  Gesells.  d.  Wiss,     Leipzig. 

Exner,  S.     Entwurf  z.  einer  physiologisch.  Erklarung. 

Lee,  F.  S.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xvii,  p.  192. 

Langley,  J.  N.,  and  Anderson,  H.  K.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xvi,  p.  4191 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xvii,  p.  27. 

Scrgi.     Dolore  e  Piacere.     Milan. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xvii,  p.  2ii. 

Mann.     Volkmann's  Sammlung  klin.  Vortrage. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Zeitsch,  f.  Heilkunde,  xvi. 

Miinzer  and  ^Viener.     Prag.  Med.  Wochenschr.,  p.  481. 

Fano,  G.     Arch.  ital.  d.  Biol.,  xxiv,  p.  438, 

Mott,  F.  W.,  and  Sherrington,  C,  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  Ivii. 


117. 

1888. 

117a. 

1888. 

118. 

1889. 

119. 

1889. 

119a. 

1889. 

120. 

1889. 

121. 

1889. 

I2ia. 

1890. 

122. 

1890. 

123. 

1890. 

124. 

1890. 

125. 

1890. 

126. 

1890. 

127. 

1890. 

128. 

1890. 

128a. 

1890. 

129. 

1891. 

130. 

1891. 

131a. 

1891. 

131- 

1891. 

132. 

1891. 

133- 

1892. 

133a. 

1892. 

134. 

1892. 

135- 

1892. 

136. 

1892. 

137- 

1892. 

138. 

1892. 

139- 

1892. 

140. 

1893- 

141. 

1893. 

142. 

1893. 

143- 

1893- 

144. 

1893. 

144a. 

1894. 

145- 

1894- 

146. 

1894. 

147. 

1894. 

148. 

1894. 

149. 

1894. 

149a. 

1894. 

150- 

1894. 

151- 

1894. 

152. 

1894- 

153- 

1894. 

iS3a. 

1895. 

154. 

1895. 

155- 

1895. 

156. 

1895. 

157. 

1895. 

BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 


399 


[895. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[896. 
[897. 
[897. 
1897. 
[897. 
[897. 

[897. 
[897. 
[897. 

:897. 
[897. 
[897. 
[897. 
[897. 
[897. 


[898. 


[899. 

[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 
[899. 


Porter,  T.  W.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xvii,  p.  455. 

Bickel,  A.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixv. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Prag.  med.  Wochenschr.,  July. 

Steinach,  E.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixiii,  p.  495. 

Gotch,  F.     Journ,  of  Physiol.,  xx,  p.  322. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Archiv  f.  exper.  Path.  u.  Pharm.,  xxxviii. 

Waller,  A.  D.     Croonian  Lect.,  Phil.  Trans.  R.  S.  London,  B. 

Gotch,  F.,  and  Burch,  G.     Phil.  Trans.  R.  S.  London. 

Lloyd  Morgan,  C.     Habit  and  Instinct.     London. 

Helmholtz,  H.  v.     Physiol,  Optik.,  2nd  ed.,  p.  916.     Hamburg, 

Goltz,  F.,  and  Ewald,  J.  R.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixiii,  p.  365. 

Gotch,  F.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xx. 

Rosenthal,  J.,  and  Mendelssohn,  M.     Neurol.  Ccntralb.,  xxi. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixv. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixviii. 

Foster,  M.,  and  Sherrington,  C.   S.     Text-book  of  Physiol.,  iii. 

London. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Lond.,  Ix,  p.  365. 
V.  Frey.     Ber.  d.  k.  sachs.  Gesells.  d.  Wiss.     Leipzig. 
Hering,  H.  E.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.     Pfliiger's  Arch.,  Ixviii. 
Hering,  H.  E.     Neurolog.  Ccntralb.,  Nr.  23. 
Hofbauer,  L.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixviii. 
Bickel,  A.     Rev.  med.  d.  1.  Suisse  romande. 
Sergi.     Zeitsch.  f.  Psychol,  u.  Physiol.,  p.  96. 
Bethe,  A.     Archiv  f.  mikros.  Anat.,  1,  p.  629. 
Bethe,  A.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixviii. 
Hering,  H.  E.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixx. 
Clifford  Allbutt.     Syst.  of  Medicine,  iii. 

Verworn,  M.     Beit.  z.  Physiol,  d.  Centralnervensystem.     Jena. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxii. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Philos.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London.,  cxc. 
Topolanski,  A.     v.  Grafe's  Archiv,  xlvi,  p.  452. 
Bethe,  A.     Archiv  f.  mikr.  Anat.,  li. 
Tschermak,  A.     Archiv  f.  Anat.  u.  Physiol.,  p.  291. 
Goldscheider,  A.     Die  Bedeutung  d.  Reize  f.  Path.  u.  Therap.  im 

Lichte  d.  Neuronlehre. 
Bayliss,  W.  M.,  and  Starling,  E.  H.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxiv,  p.  99. 
Beer,  Th.,  Bethe,  A.,  and  v.  Uexkiill,  J.    Biolog.  Centralbl.  xix, 

Nr.  15. 
Hughlings-Jackson.     Brain,  xxii,  p.  621. 
Steinach,  E.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  Ixxviii,  p.  291. 
Hering,  H.  E.     Zur  Theorie  d.  Nerventhatigheit.     Leipzig. 
Demoor,  J.     Ann.  Soc.  Roy.  d.  Sciences,  viii.     Bruxelles. 
V.  Uexkiill,  J.     Zeitschr.  f.  Biolog.,  xxxvii,  p.  381. 
Moore,  B.,  and  Reynolds,  H.  W.     Physiol.  Centralb.,  xii,  p.  501. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.    Marshall  Hall  Address,  Med.  Chir.  Trans.,  Ixxxii. 
Hering,  H.  E.     Wien.  Klin.  Wochenschr.,  Nr.  33. 
Loeb.  J.     Vergleichende  Gehirnphysiologie.     Leipzig. 
Meltzer,  S.  J.     New  York  Med.  Journ.,  May  13,  20,  and  27. 
Zwaardemaker  and  Lans.    Centralb.  f.  Physiol. 


400  BIBLIOGRAPHICAL  REFERENCES 

Barker,  L.  F.    The  Nervous  System.    New  York. 

Hunt,  Reid.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  ii,  p.  396. 

Huber,  Carl  G.    Journ.  of  Morph.,  xvi,  p.  27. 

Salomonson,  J.  K.  A.  W.    De  Laar  d.  Neurones.    Amsterdam. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Prag.  med.  Wochenschr.,  Nr.  25. 

Bickel,  A.     Archiv  f.  PhysioL 

Biedermann,  W.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixxx. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Schafer's  Text-bk.  of  Physiol.,  ii. 

Schafer,  A.  E.     Schafer's  Text-book  of  Physiol.,  ii. 

Verworn,  M.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Bd. 

Verworn,  M.     Das  Neuron  in  Anat.  u.  Physiol.    Jena. 

Donaldson,  H.     Amer.  Text-book  of  Physiol.,  ii. 

Merzbacher,  L.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixxxi. 

Uexklill,  V.  J.     Zeitschr.  f.  Biologie,  xxxix. 

Baglioni,  S.     Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Bd.,  p.  193. 

Ladd,  Q.  T.     Descriptive  Psychol.,  p.  334. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  Ixvi,  p.  390. 

Bonnier,  P.     L'Orientation.     Paris. 

Mendelssohn,  M.  13th  Cong,  intern,  de  Med.  Paris,  Sect,  of  Phys- 
iology. 

Lyon,  E.  P.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  iv,  p.  77. 

Lloyd  Morgan,  C.     Animal  Behaviour.     London. 

Langcndorff,  O.  Die  physiologische  Merkmale  d.  Nervenzelle. 
Rostock. 

Chauveau.     Le  pharynx.    Paris. 

Loeb,  J.     Comparative  Physiology  of  the  Brain.     London. 

Langley,  J.  N.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxvii,  p.  224, 

Mislawski.     Centralb.  f.  Physiologie,  xv,  p.  481. 

Frbhlich,  A.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxviii,  p.  14. 

MacDougall,  W.     Mind,  x,  Nr.  37. 

Cleghorn,  A.,  and  Stewart,  C.  S.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  v. 

Griinbaunn,  A.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London. 

Winkler,  C,  and  Rynbergk,  G.  A.  van.  Akad.  v.  Wetens.  Am- 
sterdam. 

Macwilliam,  J.  A.     Brit.  Med.  Journ.,  Nov.  30. 

V.  Uexkiill,  J.     Ergebnisse  d.  Physiol.     Erster  Jahrg.,  ii,  p.  212. 

Merzbacher,  L.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  Ixxxviii. 

Griinbaum,  A.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London. 

Tschermak,  A.,  and  Koster,  G.      Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xciii. 

Johnston,  J.  B.    Journ.  of  Comparat.  Neurol.,  xii,  p.  87. 

MacDougall,  W.     Mind,  ii,  Nr.  43. 

Hering,  H.  E.     Ergebnisse  d.  Physiol.     Erster  Jahrg.,  ii,  p.  503. 

MacDonald,  J.  S.     Thompson-Yates  Lab.  Rep.,  iv,  p.  213. 

Zwaardemaker  and  Eyjkman.  Onderzoek.  Utretcht.  Ilochsch,  5, 
iii. 

du  Bois  Reymond,  R.    Archiv  f.  Physiol.,  Suppl.  Bd.,  p.  27. 

Magnus,  R.     Mitth.  Stat.  Zool.  Neapel.,  xv. 

Yerkes,  R.  M.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  vi,  p.  440. 

V.  Baeyer,  H.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.  d.  allg.  Physiol. 

Seemann,  J.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xci,  p.  318. 


199. 

1899. 

200. 

1899. 

201. 

1899. 

2oia. 

1900. 

202. 

1900. 

203. 

1900. 

204. 

1900. 

205. 

1900. 

206. 

1900. 

207. 

1900. 

208. 

1900. 

209. 

1900. 

210. 

1900. 

211. 

1900. 

212. 

1900. 

213. 

1900. 

213a. 

1900. 

214. 

1900. 

215. 

1900. 

216. 

1900. 

2 1 6a. 

1900. 

217. 

1901. 

218. 

1901. 

219. 

1901. 

220. 

1901. 

221. 

1901. 

222. 

1901. 

223. 

1901. 

224. 

1901. 

225. 

1901. 

226. 

1901. 

226a. 

.  1901. 

227. 

1902. 

228. 

1902. 

229. 

1902. 

230. 

1902. 

231. 

1902. 

232. 

1902. 

233- 

1902. 

234. 

1902. 

235- 

1902. 

236. 

1902. 

237- 

1902. 

238. 

1902. 

239- 

1902. 

240. 

1902. 

BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES  401 

V.  Monakow,  C.     Ergebnisse  d.  Physiol.     Erster  Jahrg.,  li,  p.  563. 

Babak,  E.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  xciii,  p.  134. 

Ziehen,  Th.     Lcitfaden  d.  physiolog.  Psychologie.     Berlin. 

Mtinzer  and  Wiener.     Monats.  f.  Psychol,  u.  Neurol.,  xii,  p.  241. 

Gotch,  F.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxviii,  p.  395, 

Lewandowski,  M.     Journ.  f.  Psychol,  u.  Neurolog.,  Bd.  i,  p.  72. 

Baglioni,  S.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.,  ii,  p.  556. 

Verworn,  M.     Die  Biogenhypothese.    Jena. 

Bendy,  O.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.,  ii. 

Fr5hlich,  A.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.,  ii. 

Baglioni,  S.     Centralb.  f.  Physiol.,  Nr.  23. 

Bickel,  A.     Mechanismus  d.  nervos.  BewegungsreguL     Stuttgart 

Herrick,  C.  J.     Journ.  of  Comparat.  Neurol.,  xii. 

Sherrington,   C.  S.,  and  Laslett,  E.  E.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxiz, 

p.  58. 
251a.  1903.     Woodworth,  R.  S.     Le  Mouvement.     Paris. 

Sherrington,  C.  S.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxx,  p.  39. 

Parker,  G.  H.     Amer.  Jour,  of  Physiol.,  xvi,  Nr.  i. 

Loeb,  J.     Univ.  of  California  Publication,  Physiology. 

Yerkes,  R.  M.  (with   Ayer,  J.  B.).    Amer.  Jour,  of   Physiol.,  ix, 

p.  279. 
Fane,  G.     Arch,  italien.  d.  physiologie. 
Brodmann.     Journ.  f.  Psychol,  u.  Neurolog.,  ii,  p.  79. 
Yerkes,  R.  M.     Mark  Anniv.  Volume,  Harvard  Coll.,  p.  359. 
Yerkes,  R.  M.     Harvard  Psychol.  Studies,  i,  p.  565. 
Philippson,  M.     Compt.  rend.,  Acad.  d.  Sciences.    Paris. 
Frbhlich,  A.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  xcv. 
Philippson,  M.     Bull.  Acad.  Roy.  Belgique. 
MacDougall,  W.     Mind,  xiii,  p.  473. 
MacDougall,  W.     Brain,  Part  cii,  p.  153. 
Bethe,  A.     Allg.  Anat.  u.  Physiol,  d.  Nervensyst.     Leipzig. 
Ingbert,  C.     Journ.  of  Compar.  Neurol.,  xiii,  p.  53. 
Ingbert,  C.     Journ.  of  Compar.  Neurol.,  xiii,  p.  209. 
Bonnier,  P.     Un  nouveau  syndrome  bulbaire.     Paris. 
Sternberg,  M.,  and  Latzko.     Deutsch.  Zeitschr.  f.  Nervenh.,  xxiv, 

p.  209. 
267a.  1903.     Hitzig,  E.     Archiv  f.  Psychiat.  u.  Nervenkrank.,  xxxvii. 
Magnus.     Pfluger's  Arch.,  cii,  p.  349. 
Muskens,  J.  L.     Journ.  of  Physiology,  xxxi,  p.  204. 
Gushing,  H.     Johns  Hopkins  Bull.,  xv,  p.  213. 
Kiesow,  F.     Zeitschr.  f.  Psychol,   u.   Physiol.,  xxxiii,  p.  436;    also 

earlier,  Philos.  Stud.,  xiv,  p.  567. 
Lyon,  E.  P.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xii,  p.  149. 
Storey,  T.  A.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xii,  p.  75. 
Flechsig,  P.     Ber.  d.  sachs.  Ges.  d.  Wiss.,  Teil  i,  p.  50.,  Teil  ii, 

p.  177. 
Zwaardemaker,  H.     Archives  internat.  d.  physiol.,  i,  p.  i. 
Campbell,  A.  W.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London.,  Ixxii,  p.  488. 
Biedermann,  W.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  cii. 
Bonnier,  P.     Le  sens  des  attitudes.     Paris. 
26 


241. 

1902. 

242. 

1902. 

242a. 

1902. 

243. 

1902. 

244- 

1902. 

244a. 

1902. 

245- 

1903. 

245a. 

1903. 

246. 

1903. 

247. 

1903. 

248. 

1903. 

249. 

1903. 

250. 

1903. 

251. 

1903. 

251a. 

1903- 

252. 

1903. 

253- 

1903. 

254. 

1903. 

255. 

1903. 

256. 

1903. 

256a. 

1903. 

257. 

1903- 

258. 

1903. 

259. 

1903. 

260. 

1903. 

261. 

1903. 

261a. 

1903. 

262. 

1903. 

263. 

1903. 

264. 

1903- 

265. 

1903. 

266. 

1903- 

267. 

1903. 

267  a. 

1903. 

268. 

1904. 

269. 

1904. 

269a. 

1904. 

270. 

1904. 

270a. 

1904. 

271. 

1904. 

271a. 

1904. 

272. 

1904. 

272a. 

1904. 

273- 

1904. 

274. 

1904. 

402  BIBLIOGRAPHICAL   REFERENCES 

275.     1904.     Woodworth,  R.  S.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.    Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxxi 

p.  234. 
Yerkes,  R.  M.      Biolog.  Bulletin,  vi,  p.  84. 

Yerkes,  R.  M.    Journ,  of  Comparat.  Neurol,  and  Psychol., xiv,  p.  124, 
Hyde,  J.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  x,  p.  236. 
Rothmann.     Archiv  f.  Psychiat.,  xxxviii,  Mft.  3. 
Frohlich,  A.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  cii. 
Grosser,  Otto.    Centralb.  f.  d.  Grenzgebiete  d.  Medic,  u.  Chirurg., 

vii,  p.  25. 
Harris,  W.     Journ.  of  Anat.  and  Physiol.,  p.  399. 
Rynberk,  G.  A.  van.     Petrus  Camper.,  iii,  p.  137. 
Baglioni,  S.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.,  iv,  p.  113. 
Frohlich,  Fr.     Verworn's  Zeitschr.,  iv. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Physiol.  Soc,  March,  Journ.  of  Physiol., 

xxxi,  p.  xvii. 
Baglioni,  S.     Archivio  d.  Fisilogia,  i,  p.  575. 
Rynberk,  G.  van.     Arch.  d.  Farraacol.  Sperim.  e  Sci.  aft,  iii,  7. 
Sargent,  P.  E.     Bull,  of  Compar.  Zool.,  Harv.  Coll.,  xlv,  iii,  p.  131. 
Carlson.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  ci. 
Hyde,  I.     Amer.  Journ.  of  Physiol.,  x. 
Baglioni.     II  policlinico.     Roma. 
Pari,  G.  A.     Arch,  italien.  de  Biologic,  xlii. 
Bethe,  A.     Deutsch.  Medizin.  Wochenschr.,  Nr.  33. 
Pawlow,  J.  P.     Ergebn.  d.  Physiol.     3ter  Jahrg.,  i,  p.  177. 
Philippson,  M.     VI.  Congr.  intern,  d.  physiol.     Bruxelles. 
MacDougall,  W.     Brit.  Jour,  of  Psychol.,  i,  p.  114. 
Warrington,  W.  B.,  and  Griffiths,  F.     Brain,  xxvii,  p.  297. 
Tschermak,    A.     Institut   imperiale    de    Medecine  experimentale. 

Petersbourg. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Brit.  Journ.  of  Psychol.,  i. 
Edinger,  L.     Vorlesungen  u.  d.  nervosen  Centralorg.     Leipzig,  ii. 
Sherrington,  C.   S.     Brit.   Ass.  f.   Adv.   of  Sci.  Rep.,  Cambridge. 

Address  to  Sect,  of  Physiology. 
MacDougall,  W.     Proc.  Psychol.  Soc,  Journ.  of  Psych.,  Nr.  3. 
Langelaan,  J.  W.     Kounik.  Akad.  v.  Wetens.     Amsterdam. 
Beevor,  C.  E.     Croonian  Lectures  R.  Coll.  of  Physicians.     London. 
Biedermann,  W.     Pfliiger's  Archiv,  cvii,  p.  i. 
Sherrington,  C.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  Ixxvi,  p.  161,  and  269, 

and  previous  "  Notes  "  in  Iii,  liii,  Ix,  Ixi,  Ixiv,  and  Ixvi. 
Yerkes,  R.  M.     Pfluger's  Archiv,  cvii,  p.  207. 
MacDonald,  J.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Lond.,  Ixxvi. 
Macallum,  A.  B.     Journ.  of  Physiol.,  xxxii,  95. 
Bottazzi,  F.     Gaz.  internaz.  d.  Medicina.,  viii,  April. 
Philippson,  M.     Heger's  Trav.  d.  Laborat.  d.  physiol.,  vii,  Nr.  2. 
Kalischer,  O.     Abh.  d.  kon.  preuss.  Akad.  d.  Wiss. 
Edkins,  J.  S.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London,  Ixxvi. 
Lewandov^ski,  M.     Verhandl.  d.  physiol.  Gesellsch.  z,  Berlin. 
Sowton,  S.  C.  M.,  and  Sherrington,  C.  S.     Brit.  Med.  Journ., 

Report  on  Chloroform. 
314.     1905.     Mott,  F.  W.     Ophthalm.  Soc.  Trans.,  xxv,  p.  ci.    London. 


276. 

1904. 

277. 

1904. 

278. 

1904. 

280. 

1904. 

281. 

1904. 

282. 

1904. 

283. 

1904. 

284. 

1904. 

285. 

1904. 

286. 

1904. 

287. 

1904. 

288. 

1904. 

289. 

1904. 

290. 

1904. 

291. 

1904. 

292. 

1904. 

293. 

1904. 

294. 

1904. 

294a. 

1904. 

295- 

1904. 

296. 

1904. 

297. 

1904. 

298. 

1904. 

298a. 

1904. 

299. 

1904. 

299a. 

1904. 

300. 

1904. 

301. 

1904. 

301a. 

1904. 

302. 

1904. 

303- 

1905. 

304- 

1905. 

305. 

1905. 

306. 

1905- 

307. 

1905. 

308. 

1905. 

309. 

1905. 

310. 

1905. 

3"- 

1905. 

312. 

1905. 

313- 

1905. 

INDEX 


Actiniattt  129,  314. 
Action-current  of  nerve,  70. 
Adaptation,  324. 

the  cerebrum  and,  388,  389. 
Adapted  reactions,  reflexes  as,  235-269, 

306,  352,  389. 
Adequate  stimulus,  12,  13,  91,  245,  345. 
Affective  tone,  231,  255,  263,  260,  319, 

327.  zzo-zzz- 

Afferent  arc,  7,  55,  107. 
After-discharge,   14,  26-35,  5i>  74.  102, 

103,  288,  289,  386. 
After-image,  35,  386. 
Aiptasis  saxicola,  129,  314. 
Albertoni,  272,  397. 
Allbutt,  Clifford,  72,  399. 
Allied  reflexes,  119,  120-135,   143,  168, 

290,  348,  355.  356. 
All-or-nothing  prmciple,  71,  72. 
Alternating  reflexes,  144,  200-203. 
Ameiurus,  129,  313. 
Anaesthetics,- 14,  80,  81. 
Anderson,  Langley  and,  38,  398. 
Anelective  receptors,  227,  316,  318. 
Anger,  260,  261. 
Anodon,  85. 
Antagonistic  muscles,  84,  200-202,  279- 

284. 
Antagonistic  reflexes,  135-149,  188-190, 

203,  204,  229,  290,  322,  355. 
Anthropoid  apes,  motor  cortex  of,  272- 

279,  288,  290. 
Anthropomorphic  interpretation,  236. 
Anticipatory  reactions,  326-333,  346. 
Aorta,  99. 
Apathy,  18,  232. 
Arachne,  239. 

Arthropoda,  41,  84,  239,  315. 
Asphjrxia,  79,  80. 
Associative  recall,  331,  332,  352. 
Astacus,  85,  105,  238,  250,  295,  329. 
AsteriaSf  239. 
Atrophy,  247. 
Atropin,  305. 
Attention,  234. 
Attitude,  302-304, 336-344,  351 ;  and  see 

"  Postural  reflexes." 
Aubert,  372,  374,  375,  377,  396. 
Auerbach's  plexus,  64. 


Aurelia  aurita,  169. 
Autonomic  system  (Langley),  318. 
Axis-cylinder,  fluidity  of,  16,  17. 
Axones,  41. 

Babak,  E.,  401. 

Babuchin,  38,  396. 

V.  Baeyer,  14,  79,  400. 

Baglioni,  S.,  67,  69,  71,  80,  107,  117,  132, 

205,  401,  402. 
"Bahnung,"  14,  175-178,  184,  185. 
Ballance,  C,  272. 
Barblets,  129,  322. 
Barker,  L.  F.,  400. 

Barometic  pressure  and  sensation,  339. 
Bastian  C,  247,  270,  396. 
Bayliss,  W.,  113,  398. 

and  Starling,  3,  236,  312,  399. 
Beagle,  330, 
Beaunis,  396. 
Bee,  250. 
Beer,  Th.,  399. 
Beevor,  C,  272,  284,  398,  402, 

and  Horsley,  272,  276,  278,  290,  398. 
Behavior  of  animals,  237. 
Bell,  C,  38,  270,  287,  395. 
Bell-Magendie  law,  38,  79. 
Belmondo,  142,  398. 
Bergmann,  79. 
Bernard,  C.,  107. 
Bethe,  A.,  14,  18,  39,  41,  42,  ^i,  82,  118, 

238,  250,  399,  401,  402. 
Bezold,  A.,  398. 
Bichat,  256,  395. 
Bickel,  A.,  238,  399,  400. 
Biedermann,  W.,  9,  30,  71,  79,  85,  105, 

182,  400. 
Bile-duct,  11. 
Binocular  brightness  compared  with  uni- 

ocular,  371,  374-375- 
Binocular  contrast  compared  with  uni- 

ocular,  376-377. 
Binocular  flicker,  ,357-383 ;  symmetrical, 

362-368;  asymmetrical,  368-371. 
Binocular  fusion,  357-385. 
du  Bois-Reymond,  17,  38. 

R.,  400. 
Bonnier,  P.,  340,  342,  400,  401. 
Bottazzi,  F.,  402. 


404 


INDEX 


Bowditch,  H.  P.,  175. 

and  Warren,  175,  398. 
Bowlby,  A.,  247, 
Brachioplegia,  278. 
Brachiopod,  334. 
Brain,  dominance  of  the,  308-353. 

integration  by  the,  352-353. 

relation     to     "  distance-receptors," 
349-353. 

organ  for  adaptation  of  reactions, 

387-390- 
Breuer,  J.,  99,  396. 
Broca's  centre,  290, 395. 
Brodmann,  279,  40.. 
Brondgeest,  304. 
Bruns,  L.,  247,  398. 
Brunton,  Lauder,  193,  397. 
Bubnoff  and  Heidenhain,  175,  279,  281, 

397- 
Burch,  Gotch  and,  66,  399. 

Cajal,  Ramon  y,  15,  24,  141,  145,  232, 

279.  377,  383,  385- 
Campbell,  A.  W.,  279,  401. 
Carcinus^  15,  239. 
Carlson,  402. 
Carmarina,  118. 
Cayrade,  396. 

Central  sulcus,  genua  of,  276. 
Centrality,  significance  of,  in  the  nervous 

system,  113,  312-313. 
Centres,  rank  of,  314. 
Cercopithecus  caJlithrix,  297. 
Cerebellum,  ganglion  of  proprio-ceptive 

system,  347-349- 
Cerebral  cortex,  175,  270-309,  352-353, 
356,  383-389. 
employs  reciprocal  innervation,  279- 

299. 
genesis  of,  349. 

inhibitory  influence  of,  280-299. 
interrelation  of  the  two  hemispheres, 

289-290,  291,  384-385. 
modifying  reflexes,  300,  387-390. 
motor  reactions  of,  compared  with 

spinal,  290-292, 305-306. 
reinforcing  influence  of,  175,  176. 
"  silent "  fields  of,  278,  279. 
unequal    motor    representation    in, 
291-297. 
Cerebral  sulci,  variability  of,  273-276. 

not  functional  boundaries,  273. 
Chauveau,  A.,  182,  400. 
Chelae,  329. 

Chemo-receptors,  314,  317,  319,  325,  326. 
Chick,  331. 
Child,  254,  257,  332. 
Chimpanzee,  motor  cortex  of,  272-279. 
Chloroform,  80,  81,  113,  254,  280. 
Chordatay  320. 


Chromatolysis,  247. 
Cleghorn,  A.,  400. 
Clonic  action,  65. 

in  flexion-reflex,  27-30,  288. 

in  scratch-reflex,  30,  199,  288. 

from  cortex,  289. 
Clover -fly,  250. 
Coelenterate,  118. 

Combination  of  reflexes,  simultaneous, 
132,  133,  149-179- 

successive,  180-234. 
Cometula,  239. 
Common  path,  55,  312,  320. 

principle  of,  1 15-149,  233,  310,  346, 

final,   140,   144,    145,  233,  346,  355, 

385- 
Common  paths,  profusion  of,  144-146. 
Comparative  psychology,  307. 
Compensatory  reflexes,   144,  200,   203- 

205,  214,  337,  341. 
"  Complication,"  128,  129,  314,  347,  352. 
Conation,  331-333. 
Conative  feeling,  331-332. 
Condensation   of  receptor  distribution, 

346. 
Conduction,  6,  8. 

intracellular  and  intercellular,  4,  16, 

17,  18. 
in  nerve-cell,  2,  16. 
in  nerve-trunks,  2,  13-18,  79,  80. 
in  reflex-arc,  8,  14-18,  34,  35,  79,  80. 
reversible  and  irreversible,  14,  18. 
speed  of,  18,  19. 
synaptic,  17,  18. 
Conductor,  7,  309. 
Conjunctival  reflex,  238. 
Consummatory  reactions,  329-333. 
Contracture,  248,  303,  304. 

hemiplegic,  248,  303,  304. 
Contrast,  208,  387. 

Coordination,   contribution    to,  by    re- 
ceptor, 13. 
Coordination  in  the  simple  reflex,  8,  36- 
118. 
in  the  compound  reflex,  114-234. 
in   the  simultaneous  compound  re- 
flex {rt^cx  pattern)  150-180. 
in  the  reflex  sequence,  181-234. 
Corda  tympani,  85. 
Corneal  reflex,  238. 

Cortex,  cerebral,  see  "  Cerebral  cortex.'* 
Crayfish,  85,  105,  238,  250,  295,  329. 
Croak-reflex,  10. 

Crossed  extension-reflex  of  leg,  30,  72, 
77.  78,  79,  83,  93,  99-102,  108,  121, 
127,  135-137.  140,  143.  147,  152, 
161,  162,  164,  165,  189-191,  206- 
209,  222,  224,  225. 
Crossed  stepping-reflex,  189,  190,  224. 


INDEX 


405 


Curare,  113. 

Gushing,  H.,  277. 

Cushny,  A.,  11 1. 

Cybulski  and  Zanietowski,  70,  398. 

V.  Cyon,99,  113,  193.396. 

Darkness  in  visual  field,  367-368. 
Darwin,  C,  10,  235,  250,  257,  260,  306, 

396. 
Darwin  and  reflex  purpose,  i,  10,  235, 

236,  306. 
Decerebrate   rigidity,  89,  251,  299-305, 

338.         , 

Defaecation,  300. 

Degeneration,  Wallerian,  18. 
method  of  successive,  52-54. 

Deglutition,  69,  99,  181,   182,  322,  326, 
329,  333. 

Deiter's  nucleus,  343. 

Dejerine,  278. 

Demoor,  J.,  399. 

Dendrites,  39,  41- 

Depressor  nerve,  99,  113. 

Descartes,  256,  286,  287,  395. 

Diaphragm,  67,  68,  205. 

Diaschizis,  246. 

Differentiation    and    integration    corre- 
lated, 344. 

Diffuse  nervous  system,  311-312. 

Disgust,  261-263. 

Distance-receptors,    306,    324-328,   330, 
333-335,  390. 
the  cerebrum  the  ganglion  of,  349- 
353,  390. 

Disynaptic  arc,  54. 

Donaldson,  H.,  144,  145,  152,  334,  400. 

Double-joint  muscles,  106,  iii,  283. 

Double-sign  reflexes,  83,  135,  199. 

Duchenne,  161,  396. 

Duval,  24. 

Earthworm,  182,  328. 
Echinus,  112,  314. 
Eckhard,  C,  9. 
Edinger,  L.,  349,  402. 
Edkins,  J.  S.,  3,  402. 
Effective  threshold,  309. 
Effector,  7,  309. 
Ehrlich,  P.,  141,  232,  236. 
Elasmobranch,  349. 
Emotional  reaction,  255-268. 
End-eflfect,  6. 

rhythm  of,  14,  42-66. 

intensity  of,  14,  70-79. 
End-plate,  55. 
Erb,  396. 
Esox,  327. 

Ewald,  J.  R.,  205.  246,  338,  398. 
Excitability,  selective,  13. 


Exner,  S.,  15,  21,  87,  124,  133,  175,  181, 

279,  285,  396,  397,  398-     „ 
"  Extensor-thrust,"  67-69,  74,  88,  90,  91, 

93,  107,  146,  I47»    169,   174,  238, 

248. 
Extero-ceptive   field,    1 29-1 31,   316-322, 

324,  341,  345- 
Extero-reflexes,  129-133,  316-324. 
Eyeball  movements,  274,  275,  277,  279- 

281,  285-286,  289,  337,  384,  385- 
Eyelid-reflex,  45. 

Fano,  398,  401. 

Fatigue,  14,  214-223. 

Fechner,  372,  375,  380,  395. 

Fechner's  paradox,  375. 

Ferrier,  D.,  270,  271,  272,  280,  295,  396, 

397.  398. 
Fick,  A.,  70. 
Figure,  reflex,  164-168. 
Final    common   path,  55,  115-149,  223, 

233- 
relatively  indefatigable,  223. 

Flechsig,  P.,  277,  278,  279,  396,  401. 

Flexion-reflex,  19,  21-34,  71,  73.  74.  78, 
79,  83,  86,  88-98,  103,  104,  107- 
109,  127,  128,  131,  132,  134,  136, 
138-140,  143.  147,  149-152,  158- 
160,  102,  164-167,  169,  173.  *79» 
i8c,  187-191,  198,  203,  204,  206, 
208,  213-215,  224,  229,  240,  243, 
244,  248,  288,  355,  356. 

Flicker-sensation,  bmocular,  357-3°"  J 
asymmetrical,  368-37 1 ;   symmet- 


Flicker- 

Flourens,  270,  395. 

Fly,  238,  250. 

Focus  of  effect  of  a  reflex,  150,  151,  239. 

Foster,  M.,  14,  399. 

Franck.  Fr.,  19,  87,  397. 

Franz,  307. 

Fredericq,  L.,  239. 

Freusberg,  396. 

v.  Frey,  12,  13,  226,  398. 

Fritsch  and  Hitzig,  271,  273,  396. 

Frog,  10,  128,  181,  238,  239,  249,  251,322, 

327,  328,  330.  340,  342,  343,  349. 

352- 
Frohlich,  A.,  100,  253,  400,  401,  402. 

Gad,  J.,  39.  397,  398-    ^ 

and  Joseph,  15,  398. 
Gall-bladder,  11. 
Ganglia,  antennary  of  Carcinus,  15. 

spinal,  14,  320. 

sympathetic,  15,  321. 
Gaskell,  W.  H.,  193,  194,  i95.  325.  397- 
van  Gehuchten,  15,  85,  141. 


4o6 


INDEX 


Gergens,  9,  396. 
Gerlach,  1^4. 

Goldscheidfer,  A.,  17,  155,  399. 
Golgi,  C,  141,  154,  232. 
Goltz,  F.,  3,  9,  10,  146,  223,  230,  238,  240, 
241,  255,  262,  266,  396,  397,  398, 

399- 

Gorilla^  motor  cortex  of,  272-279. 

Gotch,  F.,  74,  87,  161,  398,  399,  401. 
and  Burch,  G.,  66,  399. 

Grainger,  238,  395. 

Grasshopper,  238,  250. 

Gravity,  as  a  stimulus,  316,  366-367. 

Gray-matter,  nervous,  14,  15,  22,  154, 
232-233,  321,  378;  delay  of  con- 
duction in,  19-26. 

Grosser,  O.,  402. 

Griinbaum,  A.  F.,  272,  274,  275,  288,  290, 
400. 

Grutzner,  P.,  92,  397. 

Gustatory  organs,  326. 

Hairs,  48,  163,  252,  341. 

Hall,  Marshall,  9,  240,  395. 

V.  Haller,  A.,  12,  395. 

Harris,  272,  395. 

Harris,  W.,  402. 

Hartmann,  383. 

Hartwell,  Newell  Martin  and,  172,  397. 

Harvey,  W.,  232. 

Haycraft,  J.  B.,  10,  398. 

Head,  H.,  67.  398. 

diaphragmatic  slip,  67,  68. 
Head,  physiological  conception  of  the, 

335-336. 
Heidenhain,  R.,  304,  397. 

Bubnoff  and,  175,  279,  281,  397. 
V.  Helmholtz,  19,  399. 
Hemiplegic  contracture,  248,  303,  304. 
Herbart,  129,  314,  395. 
Hering,  E.,  99,  19^,  196,  208,  368,  375, 

382,  384,  396,  397. 
Hermg,  H.  E.,  107,  250,  281,  283,  284, 

398,  399- 
Herrick,  C.  J.,  129,  313,  322,  401. 
"  Higher  "  and  "  lower "  in  regard   to 

organisms,  236,  237. 
Hitzig,  273,  277,  396,  401. 

and  Fritsch,  270,  271,  396. 
Hofbauer,  L.,  399. 
Homarus,  329. 
Horsley,  v.,  272. 

Beevor,  C.,  and,  276,  278,  290,  398. 
Huber,  C.,  400. 
Hughlings-Jackson,  270,  289,  303,  304, 


314,  396.  399- 
Reid,  400. 


Hunt, 

Hunter  J.,  143,  313. 
Hyde,  Ida,  402. 
Hydra,  310. 


Immediate  (or  direct)  spinal  induction, 

76,  1 1 9-1 32,  184,  185,  386. 
Incremental  reflex,  22-25. 
Indifference  between  reflexes,  146-147. 
Induction,  spinal,  immediate  (or  direct), 

76,  1 19-132,  184,  185,386. 
successive   (or  indirect)     151,  206- 

213,  387. 
Ingbert,  145,  334,  401. 
Inhibition,  14,  26,  65,,  83-106,  133-148, 

1^9-160,   191-199,  241,  327,  367, 

389- 

J.  S.  Macdonald  on,  196-198. 

from  cortex,  279-299. 

of  knee-jerk,  88-89. 

reciprocal,  83-105. 
Initial  reflex,  24. 
Injury-current  of  nerve,  17. 
Insect,  334. 
Integration,  by  nervous   agency,    2,  3, 

308-353- 
Intensity  of  reflex,  grading  of,  70-79,  93, 

150-154,  231-232. 
Intensity  of  stimulus,  14,  27,  28,  49,  150, 

152. 
effect  on  refractory  phase,  49. 
effect  on  fatigue,  219,  220. 
effect  on  prepotence  of  reflex,  223- 

225. 
Interaction  of  reflexes,  1 14-149. 

partial,  142,  143. 
Intercellular  conduction,  4. 
Intercostal  movement,  172. 
Interference  between  reflexes,  135-140, 

142,   146-149.  190-194,  I99»  232, 

310,  311,  389. 
reversible,  140,  i90-i9r. 
Internal  capsule,  280-281,  284. 
Internuncial  paths,  116,   144,  145,  328, 

329,  343,  349. 
Intero-ceptive  field,  317-318,  320,  321, 

324,  344,  345. 
Intestinal  peristalsis,  312. 
Intracellular  conduction,  4. 
lon-proteid,  196,  197,  198. 
Irradiation,  reflex,  150-170,  175-180. 
Irreversible  conduction,  14,  18,  38-42. 


Jackson,  Hughlings,  270,  289,  303,  304, 

3i4»  396,  399-  ,       „ 

James,  W.,  39,  258,  259,  265,  384,  397. 

"  law  of  forward  conduction,"  38,  39. 
Jamin,  247. 
Jaw,  innervation  of  movements  of,  290- 

292,  295-298. 
Jendrassik,  175,  397. 
"Jerk"  phenomena,  86,  89,  132,  212,  247, 

278,  302,  338. 
Johnston,  400. 


INDEX 


407 


Joseph,  Gad  and,  1 5,  398. 
Jurin,  372. 


Kalischer,  157,  402. 

Ketten-reflexe  (Loeb)  182,  335. 

Kicking,  306, 

Kiesow,  226,  401. 

Knee-jerk,  86-89,  132,  212,  247,  278,  302, 

**  Knock-out  "  blow,  343. 

Koster,  and  A.  Tschermak,  99,  1 13,  400. 

Krause,  276. 

Kronecker,  H.,  45,  99,  182. 

and  S.  J.  Meltzer,  99,  182,  298,  397. 

and  W.  Stirling,  45,  396. 
Kuhne,  W.,  38. 


Labyrinth,  133,  204,  246,  334,  335,  336- 

344.  348. 
Lacerta,  322. 
Ladd,  G.  T.,  2155,  400. 
Lange,  258,  259,  265,  397. 
Langelaan,  J.  W.,  402. 
Langendorff,  O.,  92,  349,  396,  398. 
Langley,  14,  318,  400. 

and  H.  K.  Anderson,  38,  398. 
Lans,  Zwaardemaker  and,  45,  400. 
Laryngeal  nerve,  superior,  100. 
Laslett,  E.  E.,  10,  50,  54,  401. 
Latent  period,  14,  18-26,  92. 
Latzko  and  Sternberg,  254,  401. 
Lauder  Brunton,  193. 
Law  of  Bell  and  Magendie,  38,  79. 
Law  of  forward  direction  of  conduction 

(James),  38,  39. 
Law  of  Talbot,  371,  372,  379,  380. 
Laws   of    reflex-action,   of    Pfliiger,  76, 

161-164. 
Lee,  F.  S.,  205,  398. 
V.  Lenhossek,  85,  141. 
Lewandowsky,  248,  304,  349,  401,  402. 
Lewis,  Mitchell  and,  175,  397. 
Lloyd  Morgan,  237,  265,  268,  331,  390, 

399.  400. 
Localization  in  motor  cortex,  270-307. 
"Local  sign"  in  reflexes,  124-127,  248- 

251. 
Lockjaw,  295-299. 
Locomotion,  64,  68,  69,   212,   213,  305, 

334-336,  344.  350- 
and  receptive  range,  334-336. 
Loeb,  J.,  18 r,  205,  335,  399,  400,  401. 
Lombard,  W.,  175,  397,  398. 
Lotze,  235. 
"Lower"  and  "higher"  as  applied  to 

organisms,  236-237. 
Luciani,  L.,  272,  304,  349. 
Ludwig,  C,  159. 
Lyon,  205,  400,  401. 


Macallum,  A.  B.,  198,  402. 

Macdonald,  J.  S.,  17,  68,  196-198,  199, 

400,  402. 
Macdougall,  W.,  200-203,  223,  367,  373, 


382,  383,  386.  400,  401,  402. 
illiam,  J.,  400. 
Magendie,  38,  270,  395. 


Magnus,  R.,  63,  400,  401. 

MalapteruruSy  38,  66,  74. 

Man,  motor  cortex  of,  276. 

Mann,  304,  398. 

Mann,  G.,  272. 

March  of  spinal  and  cortical  reaction, 

289. 
Marey,  E.,  45,  396. 
Mark  time  reflex,  210-212. 
Martin,  Newell,  172,  397. 
"  Material  me,"  324. 
May,  Page,  400. 
Median  line,  reflexes  of,  322. 
Median  sagittal  plane,  relation  of  cortical 

reactions  to,  289,  384-385. 
Medusa,  18,  39-42,  45,  50,  61-64,  69,  168, 

169,  249,  250,  312. 
Meltzer,  S,  J.,  99,  182,  397,  399. 

and  Kronecker,  H.,  298, 397. 
Membrane  at  cell-junctions,  16,  17. 
Membrane,  synaptic,  42. 
Memory,  228,  331,  332,  352. 
Mendelsohn,  M.,  400. 
Merzbacher,  L.,  71,  400. 
Mesencephalo  spinal  path,  329,  330. 
Metamerism,    314-316,    320,    321,    344, 

345- 
Mtlteu  interne,  4. 
Mimetic  movement,  354-367. 
Minimum  vistbile,  185. 
Mislawski,  205,  400. 
Mitchell  and  Lewis,  175,  397. 
v.  Monakow,    54,    136,    246,    273,    278, 

401. 
Monkey,  hand  of,  329. 
Monti,  24. 

Moore,  B.,  and  Reynolds,  15,  399. 
Morgan,  Lloyd,  237,  265,  268,  331,  390, 

399,  400. 
Mosso,  A.,  182,  396. 
Moth,  327. 
Motor  area  of  cerebral  cortex,  271-306, 

384,  385.  389. 
Motor  neurone,  55,  309. 
Mott,  F.  W.,  272,  279,  281,  289,  329,  385, 
389.  398,  402. 

and  Schafer,  E.  A.,  281,  289. 
Muller,  J.,  377,  383,  39V 
Munk,  H.,  272,  397,  398. 

and  Obregia,  280,  398. 
MUnzer  and  Wiener,  398,  401. 
Muskens,  205,  401. 
Mustelus,  329. 


4o8 


INDEX 


Nagel,  W.,  i8,  129,  205,  313,  398. 
Nansen,  F.,  85,  397. 
Nerve-net,  39-41,  62,  314. 
Neuroglia,  14. 
Neuromuscular  cells,  309. 
Neurone,  motor,  55,  141,  142. 

threshold,  17,  1^5,  156. 
Neurones,  amoeboid  movement  of,  24. 
Neutrality  of  reflexes,  146,  147,  289,  290, 

306. 
Newell,  Martin,  172,  397. 
Newton,  I.,  377,  395. 
Nicotin,  14,  15. 
Nissl,  247. 
Noci-ceptive  reflexes,  91,  226-230,  248, 

252-254,322,  330-332,  345. 
Noci-ceptors,  13,  226-230,  318,  330,  345. 
Nothnagel,  65,  254,  396. 

Obregia,  Munk  and,  280,  398. 

Oddi,  142,  398. 

Oesophageal  reflex,  182. 

Oestrum,  263. 

Oldag,  92,  398. 

Olfacto-phrenic  arc,  351. 

Opening  of  mouth,  252,  295-298. 

Ophioglypha,  1 1 2. 

Optic  chiasma,  377,  383. 

Optic  nerve,  70,  145,  334. 

Orang-outangs  motor  cortex  of,  272-279. 

Otocyst,  168,  169,  334,  335,  336,  337,  340, 

^      T  343- 
Ott,  I.,  397. 
Oxygen,  14,  79. 

Pain-endings,  226-229,  319. 
Pain  nerves,  226-229,  251,  252. 

path  in  spinal  cord,  251--254. 

skin,  223-228. 

visceral,  11. 
Pallio  spinal  path,  329,  330. 
Paneth,  272. 
Panum,  382. 

Parallelism  of  ocular  axes,  352,  384,  385. 
Paralysis,  after  cortical  lesion,  277,  278. 
Parasites,  63,  184,  238. 
Pari,  G.,  71. 
Parker,  G.  H.,  128,401. 
Path,  final  common,  11 5-149. 
Path,  principle  of  the  common,  1 15-149, 
310. 

private,  115,  116. 
Pattern,  reflex,  164-171. 
Pawlow,  J.,  402. 
Perceptual  image,  347,  356,  357. 
Perikarya,  14,  15,  22,  82,  83. 
Permeability  of  synaptic  membrane,  42. 
Perspective  figures,  171. 
Pfluger,  E.,  76,  161,  235,  395. 


Pfluger's  laws,  76,  161-164. 
Philippson,  68,  238,  401,  402. 
Photo-receptors,  323,  333,  337,  347. 
Phrenic  neurones,  244. 
Phrenic  reflex,  205. 
Pigeon,  343. 
Pilomotor  nerves,  261. 
Pinna-reflex,  10,  91. 
Piotrowsky,  85,  105,  398. 
Pluricellular  conductor,  39. 
Plurireceptive  summation,  123-127,  309, 

310. 
Plurisegmental  discharge,  159. 
Plurisegmental  integration,  314,  315,  344, 

345- 
Polarized  conduction,  39. 
Polimanti,  O.,  398. 
Porter,  Townsend,  244,  399. 
Post-central  convolution,  272-275,  277, 

279,  329. 

Postural    reflexes,    204,   230,   231,   337- 

345- 
Postures,  segmental  and  total,  327,  342- 

.  343.  345»  346. 
Poteriodendron,  6,  310. 
Pouting,  254. 

Pre-central  convolution,  272-280. 
Precurrent  reactions,  326,  329-330. 
Prepotent  reflexes,  224,  228-231,  319. 
Prevalence  of  contours,  367,  376,  381. 
Principle  of  the  common  path,  1 15-149, 

233,  310,  346,  351,  385. 
Principle    of    competition    for    energy 

(Macdougall)  200-203,  367. 
Proprio-ceptive  field,  129,  130,  204,  205, 

316,  317,  320,  321,  336-345,  347- 

349. 
Proprio-ceptive    reflexes,    129-132,   204, 

305,  316,  317,  320. 
Proprio-ceptive    system,    336-345,  347- 

349- 
includes  labyrinth,  336-345. 
Proprio-ceptors,  130,  131,  320,  336-345. 
Proprio-spinal  nerve-tracts,  52-54. 
Pseudaifective  reflexes,  251-254. 
Psycho-physical  parallelism,  386. 
Pulmono-phrenic  arc,  351. 
Purpose  in  reflexes,  235-239,  305. 
Purring,  255. 
Pyramidal  tract,  329,  330. 

Ramon  y  Cajal,  15,  24,  85,  141,  145,  232, 

280,  377,  383.  385- 

Reception,  6,  9-13,  309,  310,  316,  318, 

319,  323,  324. 
Receptive  fields,  46,  90, 126-131,  157, 160, 
174.  316-322. 
extero-ceptive,  130, 131, 317-319. 320» 

321,  322,  343. 
intero-ceptive,  317,  318. 


INDEX 


409 


I 


Receptive    fields,    proprio-ceptive,   129, 
130,  204,  205,  316,  317,  320,321, 

336-345.  347-349- 
of  extensor-thrust,  127. 
of  flexion-reflex,  90, 91,  128, 131,  132. 
of  scratch-reflex,  46,  121,   126,   128, 

13^  132- 
not  identical  with  spinal  root  fields, 
174. 
Receptive  range,  333,  334. 
Receptor,  7,  9,  12,  46,  61,  309,  310,  313, 
318,  319,323.335,336,347- 
a  factor  in  co-ordination,  13. 
Receptors,  classification  of,  318-319. 
distance,  306,  324-328,  330,  333-335» 

390. 
species  of,  9-13,  130,  131,  225-229, 

318-319. 
symmetrical,  149,  322. 
Reciprocal  inhibition,  83-105. 
Reciprocal  innervation,  83,  84,  90-100, 

159- 
and  cerebral  cortex,  279-299. 
and  muscular  tonus,  304,  305. 
Reflex,  the  simple,  7,8-113;  an  artificial 
abstraction,  114,  115. 
compound,  8,  114-234. 
Reflex  action,  defined,  5. 

subjection  to  "volition,"  300,  388, 

389. 
Reflex-arc,  7,  46,   50-55,    156,  308-311, 

.    320,321; 

the  primitive,  308-311. 
Reflex  attitude,  see  "  Posture." 
Reflexes,  abdominal,  163. 

adequate  stimuli  for,  9-13. 

after-discharge  of,  30,  33,  51,  74, 
102-104,  288,  386. 

allied,  1 19-135,  167,  289,  310,  355. 

alternating,  144,  200-203. 

antagonistic,  135-149,  188-191,  205, 
229,  289,  310,  311,  356. 

as  adapted  reactions,  235-269,  306. 

chain,  182. 

compensatory,    144,    200,    203-205, 

214.  337,  341- 

croak,  10. 

crossed  extension,  see  "  Crossed  ex- 
tension reflex." 

of  double  sign,  83,  135,  199. 

extensor  thrust,  see  "  Extensor- 
thrust." 

eyelid,  45. 

figure,  164-168. 

flexion,  see  "  Flexion-reflex." 

focus  of  effect  of,  150,  151,  239. 

incremental,  22-25. 

initial,  24. 

intensity  of,  see  "  Intensity  of  reflex." 

latency  of,  14,  18-26,  92. 


Reflexes,  long,  157-166,  344. 
mark-time,  210-212. 
neutrality  of,  146,  147,  289,  298,  306. 
noci-ceptive,  226-230,  248,  252-254, 

322,  330-332. 
pattern  of,  164-168. 
pinna,  10,  91. 

postural,  204,  230,  231,  337-345. 
refractory  phase  in,   14,  44-69,  134, 

135- 
rhythmic,  36,  45-66,  136. 
rhythm  of  discharge  in,  42-66,  136, 

138. 
scratch,  see  "  Scratch  reflex." 
sequence,  of,  180-234. 
sexual,  230. 
shake,  164,  238. 
short,  157-166,321,344. 
stepping,  65,  66,  210-212. 
swallowing,  69, 99,  181,  182,  322,  326, 


329.  333- 
il. 


tail,  200,  210,  212,  223,  322. 
tonic,  231,  301-305,  338-344,  348. 
torticollis,  162. 
type,  21,  65,  127. 
union  of,  347,  355,  356. 
vasomotor,  11,  321. 
visceral,  11,  317-320,  321,  322,  333. 
whisker,  162,  ^41. 
Refractory  phase  in  reflexes,  14,  44-69, 

134,  135- 
Reinforcement,  reflex,  175-180. 
Reissner  fibre,  329. 
Renaut,  24. 

Resistance,  spinal,  109,  154-156. 
Respiratory  regulation,  205,  348,  390. 
Restriction   of  distribution  a  factor  in 

integration,  346-347. 
Retrogradation,  267. 
Reynolds  and  B.  Moore,  15,  399. 
Rkizostoma,  39,  61. 
Rhythmic  reflexes,  36,  45-66. 
Rhythmic  response  from  cortex,  295. 
Rhythmic  response  in  reflexes,  42-66. 
Richet,  C,  36,  85,  105,  397. 
Rigor  mortis,  rapidity  of  onset  of,  338. 
Rohault,  377,  395. 
Romanes,    18,  39,  41,  45,  61,   118,  168, 

169,  249,  250,  396. 
Rontgen  rays,  316. 
Rosenthal,  I.,  193,  395,  399. 
Rotating  lantern,  357-362. 
Rothmann,  402. 
van  Rynberk,  402. 

Salamonsen,  J.,  400. 
Sargent,  P.,  402. 

Schafer,  E.  A..  43,  80,  272,  273,  280, 377, 
397,  398. 
Mott,  F.  W.,  and,  281,  289. 


4IO 


INDEX 


Schalt-zellen  (v.  Monakow),  54,  136. 

Schloesser,  397. 

Schopenhauer,  255. 

Schreiber,  397. 

Scratch-reflex,  10,  20,  30,  36,  45-65,  71, 
72,  75.  76,  78,  79»  9i»  103,  119, 
120-128,  131,  135-140,  142,  143. 
145,  147,  163,  173,  179,  182,  183, 
185-192,  199,  213,  214,  216-221, 
238,  239,  244,  245,  248,  288. 

Seemann,  J.,  400. 

Segment,  nervous  integration  of  the,  319- 
322. 

Segmental  arrangement  of  nervous  sys- 
tem, 314-316. 
of  motor  cortex,  277. 

Segmental  postures,  327,  342-343,  345. 
346. 

Segmental  reflexes,  204,  230,  231,  337- 

345- 
Segments,  neural  integration  of  series 

of,  314-316,  344,  345- 
leading,  323-324. 
Selachian,  330. 

Selective  excitability,  13,  227,  316,  318. 
Self -regulation  of  respiratory  arcs,  99. 
Semicircular  canals,  336-338. 
Sensation,  projicience  of,  324,  325,  331, 

343,  390. 
Sensitivity  of  viscera,  11,  12,  318. 
Sensual  fusion,  357-386. 
Sensual  "objects,"  347,  357. 
Sensual  percept,  347,  357. 
Separation  of  cells,  15,  310. 
Sergi,  259,  398,  399. 
Setschenow,  36,  65,  395,  396. 
Sexual  reflexes,  230,  326. 
Shake-reflex,  164,  238. 
Shock,  spinal,  14,  150,  240-248,  352. 
Siluroid  fishes,  129. 
Simia  satyrus,  motor  cortex  of,  273,  276- 

279. 
Simple  reflex,  7-1 15. 
Simultaneous   combination   of   reflexes, 

150-180. 
Skeletal  muscles,  tonus  of,  304,  338-339. 
Smith-Kastner,  395. 
Snarling,  252,  255. 
Sowton,  S.  C.  M.,  80,  81,  402. 
Spallanzani,  230,  395. 
Species  of  reflex,  effect  on  prepotence, 

226-231. 
Spencer,  Herbert,  257,  344,  348,  397. 
Spinal  induction,  immediate  (or  direct), 

76,  1 19-132,  184,  185,  386. 
successive   (or  indirect),  151,  206- 

213,  387. 
Spinal  nerve-root,  afferent,  85,  170,  174, 

251,  301.  319.  320. 
efferent,  319,  320. 


Spinal  nerve-root,  co-ordination  and  the, 

170-174,  251,391. 
Splanchnic,  100,  176. 
Spode,  397. 

*•  Spontaneous  "  reflex,  1 53,  208,  209. 
Stannius,  107. 
Starling,  E.  H.,  Bayliss  and,  3,  236,  312, 

399- 
Stefani,  304. 
Steinach,  E.,  9,  14,  399. 
Stepping-reflex,  65-66,  210-212. 
Sternberg,  M.,  132,  175,  398. 

Latzko  and,  254,  401. 
Stewart,  C,  400. 
Stimulus,  adequate,  12,  13,  91. 

efficacy  of  electrical,  13,  226. 

intensity  of,  14,  27,  30,  223-225,  231. 

mass  as  a,  316. 

nocuous,  13,  91,  226-230,  248,  252- 
254,  322,  330-332. 

"objecfasa,  347,  357. 

prolongation  of,  30,  329. 

threshold  value  of,  12,  208,  323-325. 
Stirling,  W.,  36,  37,  93,  396. 

with  H.  Kronecker,  45. 
Storey,  A.,  401. 
Strychnine,  71,  106-112,  132,    154,  159, 

160,  172,  292-299,  303. 
Sulcus  centralis,  genua  of,  2^6. 
Summation,   14,  36-38,  91,  93,  119,  135, 

310-31 1,  347,  355,  356. 
Swallowing,  69,  99,  181,  182,  322,  326, 

329,  333. 
Swinton,  238,  250,  397. 
Sympathetic  system,  318. 
Synapse,  17,  18,  22,  24,  25,  42,  321. 

setting  of  the,  24,  141,  142,  321. 

different  kinds  of,  299. 

different  resistances  at,  155,  156. 

an  instrument  of  co-ordination,  140, 
141,  310,  311,  321,  328,  351. 
Synaptic  conduction,  17,  18J  42,  140-142, 

154-156. 
Synaptic  membrane,  16,  17,  42,  141-142. 
Synaptic  nervous  system,  311-314. 
Syncytia,  15. 
Synergy,  178-179. 

Tail,  200,  210,  212,  223,  322. 
Talbot's  law,  371,  372,  379,  380. 
Tamburini,  272. 

Tango-receptors,  319,  322,  335,  341. 
Teleology  and  physiology,   i,   235-269, 

306. 
Tetanus   toxin,    109-113,    160,   292-299, 

303. 
Thorndike,  307. 
Threshold,  effective,  309. 

selective,  226,  227,  318,  319. 

variability  of,  14,  y]. 


INDEX 


411 


Threshold,  of  neurone,  155,  156. 
Threshold-stimulus,  12,  309,  310. 
Tiaropsis  indicans,  118,  249,  250. 
Tonic   reflexes,    231,   301-305,    338-340, 

348. 
as  basis  of  attitude,  340-344,  348. 
Tonus-labyrinth,  133,  246,  336-344. 
Tonus,  reflex,  of  skeletal  muscles,  86, 87, 

88,  301-305.  338-340. 
Topolanski,  284,  399. 
Torpedo,  74, 
Tortoise,  238,  325. 
Touch-spots,  230,  324. 
Traube,  395. 

Trauma,  as  a  stimulus,  241-244. 
Tremor,  214. 
Trismus,  295-299. 
Troglodytes  gorilla^  motor  cortex  of,  272- 

279,  288. 
Troglodytes  niger,  motor  cortex  of,  272- 

279,  288,  290. 
Tschermak,  A.,  99,  113,  199,  236,  279, 

329,  383.  399.  402. 
and  Koster,  99,  113,  400. 
Tschiriew,  397. 
Tunicate,  62,  224. 

V.  Uexkiill,  112,  307,  314,  399,  400. 
Unity  of  a  motor  centre,  76-78. 
Uspensky,  142,  396. 

Vagi,  260,  264,  287. 

Valerius,  372,  374,  396. 

Vasomotor  reflexes,  241-243,  258,  259, 

265,321. 
Vertebrate,  62,  69,  84,  315,  320,  336. 
Verworn,  M.,  2,  14,  79,  100, 141, 195,  309, 

399,  400,  401. 
Vibrissae,  162,  252,  325,  341. 


Viscera,  sensitivity  of,  11,  12. 

reflexes  from,  69,  314,  321. 
Visceral  field,  317-318,  352. 
Vocalization,  reflex,  252,  254,  255. 
Vogt,  279. 

Volkmann,  287,  395. 
Vorticella,  6,  309,  310. 

Waller,  A.  D.,  70,  80,  87,  398,  399. 

Wallerian  degeneration,  18. 

Walton,  71,  397. 

Ward,  J.,  250,  397. 

Warren,  Bowditch  and,  175,  398. 

Warrington,  W,  B.,  318,  402. 

Wasp-larvae,  331. 

Weber,  E.  H.,  287,  376,  380,  395. 

Weber- Fechner  rule,  376,  380. 

Wernicke,  304. 

Westphal,  87,  398. 

Whiskers,  162,  252,  325,  341. 

White  rami  of  sympathetic  system,  318. 

Whytt,  R.,  240. 

Wiener  and  Miinzer,  398,  401. 

"Willed"    movements,    285,   286,   306, 

387-390. 
Winckler,  400. 
Winslow,  161. 
Winterstein,  14. 
WoUaston,  377,  395. 

Woodworth,  R.  S.,  251,  254,  295, 401, 402. 
Wundt,   15,  30,  71,  193,  199,  304,  396, 

397. 

Yerkes,  R.  M.,  307,  400,  401,  402. 

Zanietowski  and  Cybulski,  70,  398. 
Ziehen,  383,  401. 

Zwaardemaker,  69,  123,  182,  400,  401. 
and  Lans,  45,  399. 


PRINTED  IN  THE  UNITED  STATES  OF  AMEEICA 


0 


■^ 


^m^ 


QP  Sherrington,  (Sir)  Charles 

j  355  Scott 

I  S55         The  integrative  action  of 

I  the  nervous  system 


IRologlcftl 

fc  Medico. 


PLEASE  DO  NOT  REMOVE 
CARDS  OR  SLIPS  FROM  THIS  POCKET 

UNIVERSITY  OF  TORONTO  LIBRARY