Skip to main content

Full text of "International journal of comparative psychology"

See other formats


ISSN  0889-3667 
IJCPB  7(1)1-43(1994) 


IISICIP 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF 

COMPARATIVE 
PSYCHOLOGY 

Published  by 

The  International  Society 

for 

Comparative  Psychology 


VOLUME?,  NUMBER  1 


EDITOR 

Robert  N.  Hughes 

Dept.  of  Psychology 

University  of  Canterbury,  PB  4800 

Christchurch,  New  Zealand 


ASSOCIATE  EDITOR 

Nancy  K.  Innis 
Dept.  of  Psychology 
University  of  Western  Ontario 
London,  Ontario,  N6A  5C2  Canada 


MANAGING  EDITOR 

Sally  A.  McFadden 

Dept.  of  Psychology 

The  University  of  Newcastle 

Newcastle,  NSW  2308  Australia 


EDITORIAL  ADVISORY  BOARD 


Ruben  Ardila 
University  of  Columbia 
Apartado  88754 
Bogota  Colombia  S.A. 

M.  Balakrishnan 
University  of  Kerala 
Kariavattom  695581 
Trivandrum,  India 

Abelardo  Brenes 
University  of  Costa  Rica 
San  Jose,  Costa  Rica 

David  Croft 

University  of  NSW 

PO  Box  1,  Kensington  NSW 

2033,  Australia 

J.D.  Delius 

Allgemeine  Psychologie 
Universitat  Konstanz 
D-78434      Konstanz      1 
Germany 

Gary  Greenberg 
Dept.  of  Psychology 
Wichita  State  University 
Wichita,  Kansas 
67260-0034   USA 


S.  Koref-Santibanez 
Museum  f.  Naturkunde 
43  Invalidenstras.se 
Berlin  104,  Germany 

Kiyoko  Murofushi 
Ritsumeikan  University 
56-1  Toujin-kita  machi 
Kyoto  City,  603  Japan 

Anthony  Nijssen 
Universitiet  Van 

Amsterdam 
Post  bus  20218 
Amsterdam,  HE  1000 
The  Netherlands 

Marco  Poli 
University  of  Milan 
Via  Francesco  Sforza  23 
Milan,  20122   Italy 

J.  Martin  Ramirez 
Universidad  Complutense 

Madrid 
Enrique  Larreta  10 
Madrid,  28036  Spain 

A.F.  Semiokhina 
Moscow  State  University 
Moscow,  119899    Rus.sia 


L.C.  Simbayi 

Dept.  of  Psychology 

University  of  Port  Elizabeth 

PO  Box  1600 

Port  Elizabeth  6000 

Republic  of  South  Africa 

Joseph  Terkel 
Dept.  of  Zoology 
University  of  Tel  Aviv 
Ramat  Aviv 
Tel  Aviv  69978  Israel 

Ethel  Tobach 
American  Mu.seum  of 

Natural  History 
Central  Park  West  at  79th  St 
New     York,     New     York 
10024-5192  USA 

Charles  W.  Tolman 
University  of  Victoria 
P.O.  Box  1700 
Victoria  British  Columbia 
Canada  V8W  2Y2 

D.F.  Ventura 
Instituto  de  Psicologia 
University  de  Sao  Paulo 
Cx.     Postal,     Sao     Paulo 
Brazil   SP 


Jiao  Shao 

Dept.  of  Psychology 
Peking  University 
Beijing  100871    China 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL 
OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


Volume  7,  Number  1,  1994 


ARTICLES 

Flavour-Meal  Size  Conditioning  in  the  Rat  (Rattus  norvegicus): 

Failure  to  Confirm  Some  Earlier  Findings  1 

Leickness  Chisamu  Simbayi 

Light  Mediation  of  Circadian  Predatory  Behavior  in  the 

Young  Alligator  27 

Jack  A.  Palmer  and  Linda  K.  Palmer 

Portia  labiata,  a  Cannibalistic  Jumping  Spider, 

Discriminates  Between  Own  and  Foreign  Eggsacs  38 

Robert  J.  Clark  and  Robert  R.  Jackson 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY  is  sponsored 
by  the  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology,  an  affiliate  of  the  International 
Union  of  Psychological  Sciences.  In  consonance  with  the  goals  and  activities  of  the 
Society,  it  publishes  reports  of  studies  in  the  evolution  and  development  of  behavior  of 
all  species;  on  the  use  of  the  comparative  method  for  the  understanding  of  behavioral 
processes;  and  the  examination  of  the  adequacy  of  psychological  and  evolutionary 
theories.  It  accepts  articles  that  deal  with  historical  and  contemporary  relationships 
between  humans  and  other  animals;  that  elucidate  problems  of  ecological  and  behavioral 
processes  in  resource  management;  that  explicate  fundamental  concepts  about  human 
evolution;  and  that  present  research  results  and  develop  theories  about  the  development 
and  evolution  of  behavior.  Articles  on  the  contemporary  and  historical  organization  of 
knowledge  about  comparative  psychology;  promoting  public  education  on  the  evolution 
and  development  of  behavior;  and  about  the  training  of  students  in  comparative 
psychology  are  also  invited.  Book  reviews;  letters  discussing  published  articles  with 
replies  by  authors;  notices  of  new  books,  journals,  meetings  and  competitions  or  other 
matters  of  interest  to  the  readers  will  be  published  at  the  discretion  of  the  editors.  The 
Editor,  Associate  Editor  and  Managing  Editor  are  elected  by  the  Operations  Committee 
of  the  Society.  The  Editorial  Advisory  Board  is  appointed  by  the  Editors  and  the 
Operations  Committee. 

MANUSCRIPTS  should  be  submitted  in  triplicate  to  Robert  Hughes,  Editor.  See  inside 
back  cover  for  style  requirements. 

SUBSCRIPTION  inquiries  and  subscription  orders  to  institutions  and  individuals  who 
are  not  Society  members  should  be  addressed  to  International  Society  for  Comparative 
Psychology,  P.O.  Box  1897,  Lawrence,  KS  66044-8897,  USA,  Telephone  913-843-1221, 
or  Fax  913-843-1274.  Subscription  inquiries  and  subscription  orders  for  Society 
members  should  be  addressed  to  Sally  McFadden,  Managing  Editor  (E-mail  address: 
McFadden@psychology.newcastle.edu.au  or  Fax  61  49  216980). 

Subscription  rates: 

Volume  7,  1994  (4  issues)  $95.00  (outside  the  U.S.,  $110.00).  Members  of  the 
International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology  receive  their  yearly  subscription  as 
part  of  their  membership  dues.    For  membership  information  see  back  pages. 

INDEXED  OR  ABSTRACTED  IN:    Psychological  Abstracts. 

PHOTOCOPYING:  Authorizations  to  photocopy  items  for  internal  and  personal  use 
of  specified  clients  is  granted  by  the  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology 
for  users  registered  with  the  Copyright  Clearance  Center  (CCC)  Transactional  Reporting 
Service,  provided  that  the  flat  fee  of  $6.50  per  copy  per  article  (no  additional  per-page 
fees)  is  paid  directly  to  CCC,  27  Congress  St.,  Salem,  MA  01970.  For  those 
organizations  which  have  been  granted  a  photocopy  license  by  CCC,  a  separate  system 
of  payment  has  been  arranged.  The  fee  code  for  users  of  the  Transactional  Reporting 
Service  is:  0889-3667/9 1/$6.50. 

COPYRIGHT  1994  by  the  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology. 
Published  quarterly. 


ISSN  0889-3667  IJCPE8  7(  1 )  1  -43(  1 994) 


International  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology,  Vol.  7,  No.  1,  1994 


FLAVOUR-MEAL  SIZE  CONDITIONING  IN  THE 

RAT  (RATTUS  NORVEGICUS):  FAILURE  TO 

CONFIRM  SOME  EARLIER  FINDINGS 

Leickness  Chisamu  Simbayi 
University  of  Port  Elizabeth 


ABSTRACT:  A  series  of  experiments  was  carried  out  in  order  to  explore  further  the 
possibility  that  hungry  rats,  both  mature  and  weanling,  might  learn  to  associate  flavours 
with  different  sizes  of  meals  made  from  the  same  diet.  The  general  procedure  used 
involved  providing  rats  with  either  a  large  meal  (e.g.  5  gm).  usually  consisting  of  wet 
mash  with  an  added  flavour  such  as  anise,  or  a  small  meal  (e.g.  1  gm)  of  the  same  diet 
with  a  second  flavour  such  as  vanilla  added,  on  alternate  days.  Following  a  number  of 
such  discrimination  training  days,  subjects  were  given  a  two-jar  extinction  choice  test 
to  assess  their  relative  preferences  for  the  two  flavours.  It  was  originally  anticipated  that 
rats  would  come  to  prefer  the  flavour  associated  with  the  larger  meal  (i.e.,  conditioned 
appetite),  because  the  larger  meal  provided  more  calories.  However,  this  result  was 
never  obtained.  When  a  significant  preference  was  acquired,  this  was  for  the  flavour  of 
the  small  meal  instead,  (i.e.  conditioned  satiety).  The  conditioned  effects  not  only 
extinguished  very  rapidly  but  were  also  rather  elusive  at  times.  It  is  thought  that  the 
observed  conditioned  satiety  effects  were  probably  due  to  flavour-calorie  rather  than 
flavour-flavour  associations. 

A  central  theme  in  a  number  of  current  theories  of  food  intake 
control  is  the  idea  that  omnivores  such  as  rats  or  human  beings  regulate 
their  food  intake  on  a  short-term  basis,  that  is,  within  a  meal,  by 
associating  the  caloric  consequences  of  ingestion  with  the  flavour  of 
foods  (e.g.,  Booth,  1985;  Deutsch,  1987;  Le  Magnen,  1987).  This 
phenomenon  which  is  known  as  conditioned  satiety  is  considered  to  be 
very  important  because  it  means  that  the  omnivores  do  not  necessarily 
have  to  wait  until  they  have  actually  experienced  the  delayed  results  of 
digestion,  namely,  calories,  before  they  terminate  feeding.  They  can 
instead  simply  rely  on  their  previous  experience  with  familiar  flavours 


Address  correspondence  to  Leickness  Simbayi.  Psychology  Department,  University 
of  Port  Elizabeth,  P.O.  Box  1600,  Port  Elizabeth  6000,  South  Africa. 

©  1994  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology  1 


2  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

as  cues  for  satiety.  Accordingly,  inability  to  make  such  associations  may 
be  responsible  for  inducing  some  pressing  human  problems  such  as 
obesity  (Booth  &  Mather,  1978),  alcoholism  (Deems,  Getting,  Sherman 
&  Garcia,  1986)  and  drug  withdrawal  problems  (Le  Magnen, 
Marfaing-Jallat  &  Miceli,  1980). 

A  complementary  process  known  as  conditioned  appetite  which  also 
involves  learning  about  flavour-food  relationships  has  been  implicated 
as  a  food  selection  mechanism  on  a  more  long  term  basis  whereby 
omnivores  choose  their  food  after  learning  about  the  caloric  values  of 
different  foods  (Bolles,  1983;  Bolles,  Hay  ward  &  Crandall,  1981; 
Hay  ward,  1983;  Mehiel  &  Bolles,  1984;  Rozin,  1977).  It  is  thought  that 
rats  are  perhaps  adept  at  learning  about  both  of  these  types  of 
conditioned  preferences  in  much  the  same  way  as  they  acquire 
conditioned  taste  aversion  (CTA)  learning  or  bait  shyness.  In  contrast 
to  the  very  considerable  and  unequivocal  evidence  for  CTA  learning  in 
rats  which  has  been  accumulated  over  the  past  three  decades  or  so  of 
research  (see  Riley  &  Tuck,  1985),  evidence  for  positive  conditioned 
flavour  preferences,  which  include  both  conditioned  satiety  and 
conditioned  appetite,  is  relatively  less  strong  and  unconvincing.  This 
seems  rather  surprising  in  view  of  the  fact  that  the  idea  of  the 
involvement  of  such  learning  mechanisms  in  the  regulation  of  feeding 
behaviour  was  originally  proposed  as  early  as  1955  by  Le  Magnen  (see 
Le  Magnen,  1969). 

Although  there  is  mounting  evidence  in  support  of  the  idea  that  rats 
are  capable  of  learning  about  positive  flavour  preferences  (e.g.,  Boakes 
&  Lubart,  1988;  Boakes,  Rossi- Amaud  &  Garcia-Hoz,  1987;  Capaldi, 
Campbell,  Sheffer  &  Bradford,  1987;  Simbayi,  1987;  Simbayi,  Boakes 
&  Burton,  1986)  most  of  the  evidence  demonstrated  conditioned  appetite 
rather  than  conditioned  satiety.  It  is  interesting  to  note  here  that  most 
of  these  studies  had  employed  fluids  such  as  glucose,  saccharin  and 
ethanol  rather  than  solid  food  as  reinforcers.  In  contrast,  a  relatively 
small  number  of  studies  have  also  demonstrated  conditioned  flavour 
preferences  using  solid  food  in  other  animals  such  as  humans  (e.g., 
Booth,  Lee  &  McAleavey,  1976;  Booth,  Mather  &  Fuller,  1982)  and 
chicks  (e.g.,  see  Capretta,  1961;  Hogan,  1977,  1980).  These  studies 
could  be  considered  more  informative  about  conditioned  preferences  than 
those  that  used  fluids  as  reinforcers,  because  most  of  the  food  of 
omnivores  is  in  solid  rather  than  liquid  form. 

The  fact  that  the  experimental  demonstration  of  conditioned  satiety 
has  proved  to  be  rather  elusive  has  been  hitherto  acknowledged  by  Smith 
and  Gibbs  (1979).  According  to  them,  only  two  studies,  one  by  Booth 
(1972)  and  the  other  by  Booth  and  Davis  (1973),  had  successfully 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  3 

demonstrated  conditioned  satiety  learning  in  rats.  In  his  initial  study, 
Booth  (1972)  found  evidence  for  the  ability  of  flavours  to  elicit 
conditioned  satiety  in  rats.  In  this  research,  flavours  were  paired  with 
high  or  low  calorie  diets  during  training  and  presented  to  the  rats  one  at 
a  time.  During  testing,  when  the  two  flavours  were  presented  one  at  a 
time  in  isocaloric  diets,  the  "low  calorie"  flavour  was  preferred  more 
than  the  "high  calorie"  flavour.  However,  in  a  two  stimulus  preference 
test  in  which  the  flavours  were  presented  simultaneously  in  isocaloric 
diets,  the  high-calorie  flavour  was  preferred  more  than  the  low  calorie 
flavour.  This  latter  result  demonstrated  conditioned  appetite.  Similar 
findings  have  also  been  reported  by  Booth  and  Davis  (1973).  However, 
similar  studies  carried  out  by  Bolles  et  al.  (1981)  and  Hay  ward  (1983) 
were  only  able  to  demonstrate  conditioned  appetite  but  not  conditioned 
satiety.  Perhaps  even  more  interesting  was  Hayward's  findings  which 
also  showed  that  young  rats  were  capable  of  more  diverse  learning  than 
adult  rats. 

The  experiments  reported  in  the  present  paper  were  undertaken  in  an 
attempt  to  explore  further  the  positive  conditioned  flavour  preference 
effects  reported  by  Booth  (1972),  Bolles  et  al.  (1981)  and  Hay  ward 
(1983)  using  wet  solid  food  as  a  reinforcer.  To  test  the  validity  of 
Haywood's  (1983)  developmental  finding,  conditioned  flavour 
preferences  were  investigated  in  adult  rats  in  Experiments  1,  2  and  4, 
and  in  rat  pups  in  Experiment  3. 


EXPERIMENT  1 

This  experiment  was  a  partial  replication  of  experiments  carried  out 
by  Bolles  et  al.  (1981),  except  for  the  following  changes: 

a)  The  ratio  of  meal  sizes  was  increased  to  5:1  instead  of  2:1.  It  was 
hoped  that  the  ratio  increment  would  serve  to  make  the  meal  sizes 
more  easily  discriminable. 

b)  To  prevent  rats  from  learning  to  anticipate  or  predict  specific 
flavours  during  each  subsequent  training  day,  semi  random 
presentations  of  flavour-meal  size  correlations  replaced  the 
alternating  presentations  every  other  day  adopted  by  Bolles  et  al 
(1981). 

c)  Control  groups  were  added  for  which  supplementary  food  (wet  mash 
of  standard  laboratory  chow)  was  made  available  15  min  after  the 


4  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

presentation  of  small  meals.  These  control  groups  were  somewhat 
similar  to  the  Oral  Group  used  in  Bolles  et  al.'s  (1981)  Experiment 
4  and  were  meant  to  equate  the  total  caloric  consequences  of  both 
meal  sizes  and  thereby  controlling  for  any  possible  confounding 
effects  of  differential  hunger. 

Basically,  rats  were  given  experience  with  two  meal  sizes  of  the 
same  diet,  each  of  which  was  marked  by  a  distinctive  flavour  (anise  oil 
or  vanilla).  Thus,  the  two  distinctive  flavours  were  correlated  with  the 
same  pattern  of  oral  cues  and  caloric  density  but  different  caloric 
consequences:  that  is,  large  meals  yielded  more  calories  than  smaller 
ones.  The  conditioning  of  flavour  preferences  was  assessed  by  the 
degree  to  which  the  animals  tracked  the  flavours  in  an  extinction  choice 
test  when  they  were  no  longer  correlated  with  meal  size. 


METHOD 

Animals 

Twenty  male  hooded  Lister  rats  were  obtained  from  Sussex  University's 
Laboratory  of  Experimental  psychology  breeding  colony.  They  had  been 
used  previously  in  a  food  neophobia  experiment  but  had  no  experience 
with  either  the  reinforcer  or  flavours  used  in  the  present  experiment. 
The  rats  were  fed  for  only  2  h  each  day  beginning  at  1500  hrs  and  were 
food  deprived  for  the  rest  of  the  day  except  for  30  -  60  mins  beginning 
at  1000  hrs  each  morning  when  training  or  testing  was  conducted.  More 
importantly,  the  animals  also  had  some  previous  experience  with  feeding 
from  jars  and  their  mean  consumption  time  for  5  gm  of  wet  unflavoured 
mash  was  about  5  min.  The  average  age  and  weight  of  the  rats  at  the 
beginning  of  the  experiment  were  100  days  and  250  gm  respectively. 

Materials 

The  experimental  flavours,  which  were  added  to  the  chow  diets, 
were  made  by  adding  0.5  ml  anise  oil  (Sigma  London  Chemical 
Company  Limited,  Poole,  Dorset,  England)  and  2.0  ml  vanilla  (E.F. 
Langdale  Ltd.,  New  Addington,  Surrey,  England)  extracts  to  100  ml 
water.  These  flavour  concentrations  were  chosen  following  a  titration 
experiment  which  tested  for  sensitivity  of  rats  to  flavours  relying  on  food 
neophobia  as  the  dependent  variable.  Both  the  conditioning  and  testing 
diets  consisted  entirely  of  standard  laboratory  chow  (Spratt's  Expanded 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  5 

Rodent  Diet,  Spiller's  Limited,  Newmarket,  Suffolk,  England)  to  which 
some  water  was  added  and  yielded  about  2  cal/gm. 

All  conditioning  and  testing  was  carried  out  in  the  animals'  home 
cages  where  both  the  conditioning  and  testing  diets  were  presented  in 
feeding  jars  with  holes  in  the  lids  large  enough  for  the  rats  to  insert  their 
heads  in  order  to  reach  the  food.  During  testing  the  jars  were  attached 
in  pairs  to  prevent  rats  from  altering  the  position  of  each  jar  relative  to 
the  other.  In  addition,  the  pairing  arrangement  also  made  it  possible  to 
effectively  counterbalance  for  any  positional  biases. 

Experimental  design 

A  2  X  2  factorial  design  was  used  with  large  meal  flavours  (anise  vs. 
vanilla)  and  supplementation  (whether  or  not  the  animal  had  received 
supplementary  food  on  small  meal  days  during  conditioning)  as  factors. 
The  experiment  had  two  major  phases  during  which  conditions  were 
reversed.  For  each  subject  the  treatments  in  each  phase  were  identical 
except  that  the  relation  between  flavour  and  meal  size  was  reversed. 
Each  phase  consisted  of  two  8-day  conditioning  cycles,  each  of  which 
were  followed  by  a  single  test  day.  Finally,  a  5-day  interval  separated 
the  two  phases  during  which  no  flavours  were  presented  and  the  same 
food  deprivation  schedule  as  used  in  the  actual  experiment  was  also 
maintained. 

Procedure 

Conditioning  involved  presenting  flavour-meal  size  correlations  for 
blocks  of  8  days  in  semi  random  order,  that  is,  a  total  of  4  days  with 
each  flavour.  The  animals  were  randomly  assigned  to  four  equal  groups 
(n  =  5)  namely.  Groups  V,  VS,  A  and  AS.  Groups  V  and  VS  were 
presented  with  a  large  meal  (5  gm)  flavoured  with  2.0  %  vanilla  on 
some  days  and  a  small  meal  (1  gm)  flavoured  with  0.5  %  anise  oil  on 
other  days.  On  the  other  hand.  Groups  A  and  AS  were  both  presented 
with  a  large  meal  (5  gm)  flavoured  with  0.5  %  anise  oil  on  some  days 
and  a  small  meal  (1  gm)  flavoured  with  2.0  %  vanilla  on  the  other  days. 
However,  in  addition,  Groups  VS  and  AS  also  received  a  supplement  of 
4  gm  of  unflavoured  plain  chow  (also  in  wet  mash  form)  15  min  after 
consuming  the  small  meals.  On  any  given  conditioning  day,  all  the 
animals  received  the  same  flavour:  for  example,  on  a  typical  'vanilla 
day',  Group  V  and  VS  received  large  vanilla  flavoured  meals,  while 
Groups  A  and  AS  received  small  vanilla  flavoured  meals,  and  only 
Group  AS  received  the  4  gm  of  supplementary  food  after  15  min.   The 


6  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

flavoured  meals  used  for  conditioning  were  presented  at  1 000  hr  while 
maintenance  food  was  presented  for  2  h  beginning  at  1500  hr. 

Testing  was  done  using  a  two  jar  extinction  choice  paradigm  which 
involved  comparing  the  consumption  of  two  diets  presented 
simultaneously  in  equal  amounts  in  two  feeding  jars.  Each  jar  contained 
20  gm  of  either  anise  or  vanilla  flavoured  food  in  wet  mash  form. 
Testing  lasted  for  only  10  min  beginning  at  1000  hr  on  each  test  day. 
The  positions  of  test  foods  were  counterbalanced  for  each  pair  of 
animals  to  minimise  any  positional  biases.  The  first  test  was  done  on 
Day  9  (Phase  1)  after  which  the  conditioning  cycle  was  repeated  for 
another  8  days.  Then,  another  choice  test  followed  on  Day  23  (Phase  2). 

In  order  to  further  demonstrate  the  consistency  and  resilience  of  the 
phenomenon  under  investigation,  the  above  procedure  was  repeated 
exactly  except  that  the  flavours  were  reversed  during  Phase  2  as 
mentioned  earlier.  A  5-day  interval  was  allowed  to  reduce  the  chances 
of  rats  confusing  flavour-meal  size  pairings  used  in  Phase  1 .  However, 
no  tests  were  carried  out  to  ascertain  whether  or  not  preferences  had 
remained  unchanged  during  the  intervening  period.  Thus,  during  Phase 
2  Groups  V  and  VS  were  presented  with  anise-flavoured  large  meals  on 
some  days  and  vanilla-flavoured  small  meals  on  other  days  while  Groups 
A  and  AS  were  presented  with  vanilla-flavoured  large  meals  on  some 
days  and  anise-flavoured  small  meals  on  other  days.  Note  that  these 
flavour  meal  size  correlations  are  exactly  the  opposite  to  treatments 
given  in  Phase  1.  In  addition.  Groups  VS  and  AS  were  retained  as 
controls  as  in  Phase  1,  and  they  both  received  supplements  of  4  gm  of 
unflavoured  plain  mash  15  min  after  the  presentation  of  food  on  'small 
meal'  days.  However,  as  in  Phase  I,  Groups  V  and  A  did  not  receive 
any  supplementary  food  on  their  small  meal  days. 

Relative  preferences  for  anise-flavoured  food  in  the  extinction  choice 
tests  which  followed  each  8  day  training  cycle  were  calculated  in  terms 
of  percentages  of  total  food  consumption  by  each  subject  during  each 
test  as  follows: 

intake  of  anise 
intake  of  both  anise  and  vanilla 


Preference  for  anise  (%)  =  100  ^ 


Data  analysis 

Data  obtained  during  each  of  the  two  phases  of  the  experiment  were 
initially  assessed  using  separate  three  way  analyse  of  variance  (ANOVA) 
with  repeated  measures,  comprising  large  meal  flavour  (anise  versus 
vanilla),  supplementation  (supplement  versus  no  supplement)  and  test 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  7 

session  (Test  1  versus  Test  2)  as  factors.  In  order  to  determine  how 
flavour  preferences  shifted  after  each  8  day  training  cycle,  data  obtained 
during  each  of  the  two  test  sessions  in  each  phase  were  also  separately 
assessed  using  two  way  ANOVAs  with  large  meal  flavour  and 
supplementation  as  factors. 


RESULTS 

During  conditioning,  all  animals  ate  all  of  the  portions  of  food  given 
to  them  every  day.  The  data  obtained  from  the  extinction  choice  tests 
carried  out  at  the  end  of  each  of  the  four  8  day  training  cycles  in 
Experiment  1  are  illustrated  in  Figure  1 .  This  figure  shows  that  although 
all  groups  highly  preferred  vanilla  compared  with  anise  in  all  choice 
tests  (as  revealed  by  low  anise  preferences  ranging  from  2  %  to  35  %), 
quite  clear  group  differences  could  still  be  seen. 


PHASE  1 


2  1 

TEST  SESSIONS 


PHASE  2 


-  GROUP  V  -t-  GROUP  VS  -X-  GROUP  A  "■-  GROUP  AS 


Figure  1.  Mean  preferences  (%)  for  anise  flavour  during  the  two-jar  extinction  choice 
tests  in  Experiment  1  (n  =  5).  Note:  Flavour-meal  size  pairings  were  reversed  during 
Phase  2. 


8  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

The  ANOVAS  performed  on  data  obtained  during  each  of  the  two 
phases  of  the  experiment  revealed  a  significant  main  effect  of  large  meal 
flavour  during  Phase  1  only,  F(3,16)  =  5.69,  p  <  0.01.  As  can  be  clearly 
seen  in  Figure  1,  this  indicates  that  both  Groups  V  and  VS  consumed 
significantly  more  anise-flavoured  food  than  Groups  A  and  AS  during 
Phase  1 .  However,  there  were  no  such  major  flavour  preference  shifts 
observed  during  Phase  2  following  reversal  training,  although  the 
direction  of  the  small  preference  shifts  that  had  occurred  was  similar  to 
that  observed  during  Phase  1. 

A  two  way  ANOVA  of  data  obtained  during  Test  1  of  Phase  1 
revealed  reliable  main  effects  of  both  large  meal  flavour  and 
supplementation,  F(l,  16)  =  8.17  and  5.32,  p  <  0.01  and  0.05, 
respectively.  However,  there  was  no  significant  interaction  between  the 
two  factors.  As  can  also  be  clearly  seen  in  Figure  1,  supplementation  of 
meals  significantly  reduced  the  magnitude  of  flavour  preference  shifts 
during  Test  1 .  Another  two  way  ANOVA  of  data  obtained  during  Test 
2  of  Phase  1  revealed  a  significant  main  effect  of  large  meal  flavour 
only,  F(l,16)  =  5.32,  p  <  0.05.  Similar  statistical  analyse  of  data 
obtained  during  each  of  the  two  tests  in  Phase  2  of  the  experiment  failed 
to  reveal  any  significant  main  effects  or  interactions. 


DISCUSSION 

Three  main  findings  emerged  from  Experiment  1 .  Firstly,  adult  rats 
learned  to  prefer  a  particular  flavour  when  it  was  previously  correlated 
with  small  meals  more  than  when  the  same  flavour  was  correlated  with 
larger  ones.  Secondly,  supplementation  of  small  meals  initially  slightly 
reduced  the  preferences  for  the  flavour  previously  paired  with  the  small 
meal,  but  had  no  effect  on  the  preferences  afterwards.  Even  though  the 
rats  had  clearly  a  positive  preference  for  vanilla  and  what  appeared  to  be 
an  unconditioned  aversion  to  anise,  it  was  also  clear  from  the  data  that 
the  flavour  tracking  effect  was  quite  consistent  throughout  the 
experiment.  Thirdly,  although  reverse  flavour-meal  size  pairings  reduced 
the  preferences  to  non  significance,  they  were  still  in  the  direction 
determined  by  meal  sizes. 

Although  the  first  finding  appears  to  be  consistent  with  Booth's 
(1972;  conditioned  satiety  findings,  the  extinction  testing  procedure 
which  was  used  in  the  present  experiment  was  different  from  that 
utilised  by  Booth.  For  instance.  Booth's  testing  procedure  involved 
presenting  the  two  test  flavours  one  at  a  time  whereas  in  the  present 
experiment  the  testing  procedure  involved  simultaneous  (or  side  by  side) 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  9 

presentation  of  the  two  test  flavours.  However,  the  same  finding  clearly 
contradicts  reports  by  Bolles  et  al.  (1981)  and  Hay  ward  (1983)  that  adult 
rats  can  not  learn  taste  preferences  based  on  differing  caloric  outcome. 
The  second  finding  appears  to  be  entirely  consistent  with  those  of 
Booth  and  Davis  (1973)  and  Booth  et  al.  (1982)  who  found  that  although 
the  latter  supplements  failed  to  condition  meal  size  per  se,  they  did 
actually  manage  to  condition  flavour  preferences.  Finally,  the  third 
finding  could  be  explained  in  terms  of  either  overshadowing  or  proactive 
interference  (or  inhibition)  of  the  subsequent  learning  during  Phase  2  by 
learning  which  took  place  earlier  during  Phase  1. 


EXPERIMENT  2 

One  criticism  of  the  conditioned  satiety  hypothesis,  namely  that  the 
animal  will  prefer  the  flavour  not  associated  with  satiety,  is  that  it  might 
instead  avoid  the  flavour  with  a  prolonged  exposure  due  to  an  acquired 
aversion.  In  order  to  rule  out  this  possibility,  in  Experiment  2  flavours 
were  presented  for  an  equal  amount  of  time  but  were  followed  or 
preceded  by  different  sized  supplements.  Therefore,  for  one  half  of  the 
subjects,  one  flavour  was  followed  by  4  gm  while  a  second  flavour  was 
followed  by  1  gm  of  plain  mash.  Hence,  this  procedure  controlled  both 
for  flavour  exposure  time  and  for  the  disturbance  factor  involved  when 
supplements  are  given  after  the  presentation  of  flavour  cues.  Another 
important  issue  concerns  the  extent  to  which  the  relationship  between  the 
flavour  cue  and  the  caloric  supplement  could  also  be  important,  since 
cues  presented  at  meal  onset  may  have  a  different  value  compared  with 
those  at  the  end  of  the  meal.  In  order  to  investigate  this  issue  flavour 
cues  were  preceded  by  supplements  for  the  remaining  half  of  the 
experimental  subjects.  This  allowed  comparisons  to  be  made  to  examine 
the  extent  to  which  conditioned  satiety  depended  upon  the  relative 
temporal  positions  of  flavour  cues  and  supplements. 


METHOD 

Animals  and  materials 

The  subjects  consisted  of  24  male  hooded  Lister  rats  obtained  from 
the  same  breeding  colony  as  in  the  previous  experiment.  They  were 
aged  about  160  days  and  weighed  325  -  480  gm  at  the  beginning  of  the 
experiment.     The  rats  had  previously  been  used  in  another  flavour 


10  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

conditioning  experiment  employing  the  Holman  (1975)  procedure  using 
delayed  reinforcement  (Simbayi,  1987),  but  had  no  experience  with  the 
solid  reinforcer,  the  two  flavours  (i.e.,  vanilla  and  anise)  and  the 
conditioning  procedure  used  in  this  experiment.  The  subjects  were 
allowed  about  4  weeks  of  ad  libitum  feeding  prior  to  being  given  another 
2  weeks  to  adjust  to  new  housing  conditions  and  a  new  feeding  schedule 
similar  to  that  used  in  Experiment  1 .  In  addition,  they  were  allowed  6 
days  to  accommodate  to  feeding  on  wet  chow  from  feeding  jars  placed 
inside  their  individual  cages  before  the  actual  experiment  began. 

All  the  materials  used  were  similar  to  those  used  in  Experiment  1 . 

Procedure 

The  rats  were  housed,  watered,  fed  and  tested  exactly  as  in 
Experiment  1.  However,  some  major  changes  were  made  during 
training.  Discrimination  training  commenced  at  the  same  time  of  day  as 
in  Experiment  1  and  flavours  were  also  presented  in  a  semi-random 
order  for  8  days.  The  subjects  were  randomly  divided  into  four  groups 
(n  =  6),  namely.  Groups  VS,  AS,  SV  and  SA.  Groups  VS  and  AS 
received  flavoured  mash  initially  before  supplements  of  plain  mash  were 
presented  whereas  supplements  of  plain  mash  preceded  the  presentation 
of  flavoured  mash  in  Groups  S V  and  S A.  All  four  groups  received  1  gm 
of  either  vanilla  or  anise  flavoured  mash,  each  flavour  separately  for  a 
total  of  4  days.  On  any  particular  conditioning  day,  all  groups  received 
only  one  of  the  two  flavours  which  served  as  the  conditioned  stimuli. 
Group  VS  received  4  gm  of  plain  mash  2  min  after  the  presentation  of 
1  gm  of  vanilla  flavoured  mash  on  some  days  and  only  1  gm  of  plain 
mash  also  2  min  after  the  presentation  of  1  gm  of  anise  flavoured  mash 
on  other  days.  Groups  AS  received  a  similar  treatment  to  Group  VS, 
except  that  flavour-meal  size  contingencies  were  reversed,  that  is,  4  gm 
of  plain  mash  2  min  after  the  presentation  of  1  gm  of  anise  flavoured 
mash  on  some  days  and  1  gm  of  plain  mash  also  2  min  after  the 
presentation  of  1  gm  of  vanilla  flavoured  mash  on  other  days.  However, 
Groups  SV  and  SA  were  presented  with  the  same  flavour-meal  size 
pairings,  as  Groups  VS  and  AS  respectively,  except  that  the  order  of  the 
presentation  of  the  flavoured  and  plain  mash  were  reversed.  Therefore, 
Group  SV  received  4  gm  of  plain  mash  5  min  before  the  presentation  of 
1  gm  of  vanilla  flavoured  mash  on  some  days  and  also  1  gm  of  plain 
mash  2  min  before  the  presentation  of  1  gm  of  anise  flavoured  mash  on 
the  other  days.  Similarly,  Group  SA  received  the  same  treatment  as 
Group  SV,  except  that  the  flavour-meal  size  pairing  was  reversed,  that 
is,  4  gm  of  plain  mash  was  presented  5  min  before  the  presentation  of 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  11 

1  gm  of  anise  flavoured  mash  on  some  days  and  1  gm  of  plain  mash  2 
min  before  the  presentation  of  1  gm  of  vanilla  flavoured  mash  on  other 
days.  The  2-min  and  5-min  intervals  were  introduced  to  enable  the 
animals  to  eat  all  of  the  initial  plain  and  flavoured  mash  respectively 
before  additional  food  was  presented.  This  procedure  was  adopted  to 
prevent  mixing  of  flavoured  and  unflavoured  food  and  a  possible 
reduction  of  the  potency  of  the  conditioned  stimuli  due  to  dilution. 

Testing  for  changes  in  preferences  of  either  anise-   or  vanilla- 
flavoured  mash  was  performed  using  two  jar  extinction  choice  tests  on 

2  consecutive  days  and  lasted  for  10  min  on  each  day  as  in  Experiment 
1 .  Rats  were  simultaneously  presented  with  30  gm  of  vanilla  flavoured 
mash  and  30  gm  of  anise  flavoured  mash  in  separate  jars.  Both  Tests 
1  and  2  were  held  under  conditions  of  food  deprivation  similar  to  those 
employed  during  conditioning. 


RESULTS 

All  animals  consumed  their  food  portions  on  all  training  days.  The 
results  obtained  from  the  choice  tests  carried  out  at  the  end  of  the  single 
8-day  training  cycle  are  shown  in  Figure  2.  During  Test  1,  both  Groups 
VS  and  SV  which  had  prior  exposure  to  large  vanilla-flavoured  meals 
and  small  anise-flavoured  meals  had  higher  preferences  for  anise- 
flavoured  mash  than  Groups  AS  and  SA  which  had  been  exposed  to 
large  anise-flavoured  meals  and  small  vanilla- flavoured  meals.  There 
was  a  similar  pattern  of  group  preferences  during  Test  2.  During  both 
Tests  1  and  2,  Groups  VS  and  AS  (which  had  received  cues  before 
supplements)  had  higher  preferences  for  anise-flavoured  mash  than 
Groups  SV  and  SA,  which  had  received  flavour  cues  after  the  meal 
supplements. 

A  two  way  ANOVA  of  data  obtained  during  Test  1  revealed 
significant  main  effects  for  both  large  meal  flavour  and  the  timing  of  the 
supplement,  Fs  (1,  20)  =  10.43  and  6.22,  ps  <  0.01  and  0.05, 
respectively.  However,  there  was  no  significant  interaction  between  the 
two  factors.  The  former  main  effect  indicates  that  Groups  VS  and  SV 
had  significantly  higher  preferences  for  anise-flavoured  mash  than 
Groups  AS  and  SA,  whereas  the  latter  one  shows  that  flavour  cues  were 
more  effective  as  conditioned  stimuli  during  conditioning  when  they 
were  presented  at  meal  onset  as  in  Groups  VS  and  AV  than  at  meal 
offset  as  in  Groups  SV  and  SA.  When  data  obtained  during  Test  2  was 
also  statistically  assessed,  neither  the  main  effects  nor  the  interaction 
between  the  two  factors  were  found  to  be  significant. 


12 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


TEST  1 


TEST  2 


TEST  SESSION 


GROUP  VS  E]  GROUP  AS  D  GROUP  SV  ^  GROUP  SA 


Figure  2.    Mean  preferences  (%)  for  anise  flavour  during  the  two-jar  extinction  choice 
tests  in  Experiment  2  (n  =  6).    Bars  represent  standard  errors. 


DISCUSSION 


Three  main  findings  emerged  from  Experiment  2.  Firstly,  it 
confirmed  the  finding  in  Experiment  1  that  adult  rats  are  capable  of 
learning  about  flavour-meal  size  associations.  Even  when  flavour- 
exposure  time  was  controlled,  the  rats  learned  to  prefer  a  flavour 
previously  associated  with  a  small  meal  better  than  when  the  same 
flavour  was  paired  with  a  large  meal.  Secondly,  flavour-meal  size 
effects  were  stronger  when  flavour  cues  were  presented  at  meal  onset 
than  at  meal  offset.  Thirdly,  the  conditioned  effects  extinguished  very 
rapidly. 

The  finding  that  conditioned  satiety  effects  could  still  emerge  when 
flavour  exposure  time  was  controlled  provides  further  evidence  that 
satiety  associated  with  the  ingestion  of  large  meals  has  no  positive 
reinforcement  properties.  Similar  conclusions  have  been  arrived  at  by 
Van  Vort  and  Smith  (1983).     The  present  finding  also  weakens  the 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  13 

argument  that  conditioned  flavour  preferences  observed  in  Experiment 
1  were  due  to  an  acquired  aversion  towards  prolonged  flavour  exposure 
which  was  associated  with  large  meals  during  conditioning. 

The  second  finding  that  flavour  cues  were  presented  at  meal  onset 
rather  than  at  meal  offset  suggested  that  the  cues  may  be  more  salient 
and  less  interfered  with  at  the  former  than  at  the  latter  stage.  The 
presentation  of  flavour  cues  following  supplements  was  still  effective  in 
conditioning  satiety  but  it  reduced  the  effects  considerably.  It  is  possible 
that  when  supplements  preceded  flavour  cues,  they  interfered  with  the 
learning  of  the  discrimination  between  the  two  flavours  and  the 
appropriate  satiety  signals  derived  from  the  two  meal  sizes.  The  result 
of  such  interference  was  probably  some  weakening  of  the  effects  of  the 
conditioning. 

An  alternative  explanation  would  be  that  the  rats  became  satiated 
after  consuming  the  supplements  and  perceived  the  appropriate  satiety 
signals  immediately  afterwards  before  the  flavour  cues  were  even 
presented  and  perceived.  Therefore,  it  would  appear  from  the  present 
data  that  the  strength  of  any  flavour-meal  size  conditioning  is  stronger 
when  the  flavour  cues  precede  supplements  than  vice  versa.  This  finding 
appears  to  contradict  the  findings  reported  by  Booth  and  Davis  (1973) 
in  rats  and  Booth  et  al.  (1982)  in  humans,  where  the  later  supplements 
failed  to  condition  meal  size  per  se  and  conditioned  only  flavour 
preferences  at  most.  The  third  finding  that  the  conditioned  satiety  effects 
were  weak  and  extinguished  rapidly  was  surprising  when  compared  with 
the  extreme  persistency  of  the  other  types  of  flavour  conditioning  based 
phenomena  (see  Capaldi,  Myers,  Campbell  &  Sheffer,  1983;  Logue, 
1979;  Revusky,  1974;  although  see  Simbayi,  1987).  The  effects  had 
disappeared  as  early  as  the  second  test  day  after  an  8-day  conditioning 
cycle  during  which  each  flavour-meal  size  pairing  was  presented  four 
times  only.  Perhaps,  the  effects  could  have  been  more  persistent  and 
more  resistant  or  less  susceptible  to  extinction  with  additional 
conditioning. 


EXPERIMENT  3 

Both  previous  experiments  demonstrated  flavour-meal  size 
conditioning  in  adult  rats.  The  aim  of  Experiment  3  was  to  test 
Hay  ward's  (1983)  claim  that  rat  pups  are  apparently  capable  of  more 
diverse  learning  than  adult  rats.  As  indicated  earlier,  Hayward  showed 
that  rat  pups,  but  not  adult  rats,  could  acquire  a  conditioned  taste 
preference  for  a  flavour  paired  with  a  diet  that  provides  more  calories 


14  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

than  another  diet  of  equal  caloric  density.  In  particular  in  her 
Experiment  3,  she  obtained  stronger  conditioning  of  caloric  effects  with 
rat  pups  when  a  4  cal/gm  novel  diet  represented  the  major  part  of  the 
animal's  daily  caloric  intake  for  just  4  conditioning  days.  The  pups  were 
given  unlimited  access  to  20  gm  of  the  4  cal/gm  diet  on  one  day,  and 
three  quarters  of  the  previous  day's  consumption  on  alternate  days.  In 
order  to  control  for  differential  hunger  experienced  on  days  when  rats 
had  inadequate  food,  the  food  supply  of  half  of  the  animals  was  topped 
up  with  laboratory  chow  8  h  after  presentation  of  the  novel  diet.  Thus, 
the  present  experiment  was  meant  to  confirm  such  caloric  effects  by 
using  a  procedure  almost  identical  to  that  employed  by  Hayward  except 
for  the  following  four  minor  changes  which  were  made.  Firstly,  rats 
were  first  familiarised  with  the  diets  to  reduce  neophobia.  Secondly,  on 
small  days  the  rats  received  only  half  of  the  amounts  of  food  they 
received  on  large  meal  days  to  make  the  meal  sizes  more  easily 
discriminable  but,  as  indicated  above,  their  training  diets  were  also 
supplemented  by  laboratory  chow  pellets.  Thirdly,  the  duration  of 
testing  was  extended  to  cover  a  period  of  up  to  24  h  with  intervals  at  30 
min  and  4  h  instead  of  only  after  30-min  and  3-h  intervals  as  per 
Hayward  (1983).  This  allowed  monitoring  of  intake  rates  for  a  period 
longer  than  a  meal  which  in  turn  made  it  possible  for  one  to  distinguish 
between  aversions  taking  place  early  and  throughout  the  meal  as  opposed 
to  those  taking  place  later.  This  in  turn  made  it  possible  to  distinguish 
between  conditioned  aversion  and  conditioned  satiety  respectively. 
Finally,  in  order  to  facilitate  comparisons  with  other  experiments 
reported  in  the  present  paper,  conditioning  lasted  for  8  days  instead  of 
only  4  as  in  Hayward's  study. 


METHOD 

Animals 

Twenty  four  male  naive  Lister  rat  pups,  24  -  28  days  old  and 
weighing  46-91  gm  at  the  beginning  of  the  experiment,  were  used. 
The  rat  pups  were  weaned  at  the  age  of  2 1  days  old  and  reared  in  colony 
cages  (4-6  animals  per  cage).  They  had  ad  libitum  access  to  dry 
laboratory  chow  pellets  (Scientific  Foods,  Croydon,  England)  and  tap 
water  for  3  -  7  days  before  the  experiment  commenced.  Following 
selection  and  assignment  to  groups,  the  weanling  rats  were  housed 
individually  in  wire  cages  in  a  cage  rack  in  an  experimental  room  and 
had  ad  libitum  access  to  water  throughout  the  experiment. 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  15 

Materials 

The  recipe  for  the  novel  diet  used  was  identical  to  that  utilised  by 
Hayward  as  the  high  calorie  food.  It  consisted  of  50%  dextrin  (Sigma), 
15%  calcium  carbonate  (chalk,  East  Anglia  Chemicals),  2.5%  mineral  oil 
(liquid  paraffin,  British  Drug  Houses,  now  M.  W.  Scientific,  Ltd.,  Poole, 
Dorset,  England),  7.5%  ordinary  (domestic)  vegetable  cooking  oil  and 
25%  lactic  casein  (Sigma).  The  diet  provided  approximately  4  cal/gm. 
Either  vanilla  or  anise  extract  was  added  to  the  diets  separately  in  the 
following  proportions:  for  vanilla,  10  ml  of  flavour  was  added  to  100  gm 
of  the  diet  mixture;  for  anise,  0.50  ml  of  flavour  was  dissolved  in  20  ml 
of  water  and  also  added  to  100  gm  of  the  diet  mixture. 

Procedure 

The  rats  were  assigned  randomly  to  four  groups  (n  =  6),  namely. 
Groups  R-A,  R-V,  Ad-A,  and  Ad-V,  with  R  and  Ad  standing  for 
restricted  and  ad  libitum  conditions  respectively,  as  explained  below. 
Groups  R-A  and  R-V  represented  Groups  A  and  V  in  Experiment  1  in 
the  present  study  whereas  Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V  represented  Group  2 
(the  chow  group)  in  Experiment  3  reported  by  Hayward  (1983).  On 
familiarisation  days,  all  the  weanling  rats  were  exposed  to  the 
unflavoured  novel  4  cal/gm  diet.  First,  all  groups  received  40  gm 
overnight.  Then,  Groups  R-A  and  R-V  received  only  5  gm  while 
Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V  continued  to  receive  unrestricted  access  to  40 
gm  for  every  24  h  beginning  at  1200  hrs  for  the  next  3  consecutive  days. 

On  conditioning  days,  Groups  R-A  received  anise  flavour  in  5  gm 
of  the  novel  4  cal/gm  diet  on  some  days  and  vanilla  flavour  in  1  gm  of 
the  same  diet  on  other  days.  The  flavour-meal  size  pairing  was  reversed 
for  Group  R-V.  Group  Ad-A  received  anise  flavour  in  20  gm  of  the 
novel  4  cal/gm  diet  on  some  days  and  vanilla  flavour  in  10  gm  of  the 
same  diet  on  other  days.  Similarly,  the  flavour-meal  size  pairing  was 
reversed  for  Group  Ad-V.  Therefore,  for  all  rats,  the  two  flavours  were 
correlated  with  the  same  pattern  of  oral  cues  but  differing  caloric 
consequences  (i.e.,  20  vs.  4  calories  for  Groups  R-A  and  R-V  and  80  vs. 
40  calories  for  Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V,  both  respectively).  Altogether, 
rats  in  Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V  experienced  4  days  with  sufficient  food 
(i.e.,  large  meals)  and  another  4  days  with  an  inadequate  amount  of  food 
(i.e.,  small  meals).  In  order  to  avoid  the  problem  of  differential  hunger 
on  days  of  inadequate  food,  the  food  supply  was  supplemented  by  5 
pellets  of  dry  laboratory  chow  (weighing  approximately  10  gm  and 
containing  40  calories)  5  h  after  the  presentation  of  the  training  diet. 


16  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

For  both  Groups  R-A  and  R-V,  laboratory  chow  was  removed  2.5  h 
prior  to  the  presentation  of  the  novel  diet  and  returned  5  h  afterwards. 
The  remaining  rats  in  both  Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V  received  no 
additional  food  except  only  as  specified  above,  that  is,  on  small  meal 
days.  Flavours  were  also  presented  in  semi-random  order  as  in  previous 
experiments. 

All  groups  were  given  an  extinction  choice  test  between  the  anise 
and  vanilla  flavoured  diets  on  Day  9.  Two  jars  of  the  4  cal/gm  food 
were  presented  simultaneously,  and  consumption  was  recorded  at  the  end 
of  30  min,  4  h  and  24  h  for  all  four  groups.  As  in  previous  experiments, 
preferences  for  anise-flavoured  food  were  calculated  as  percentages  of 
total  food  intake. 

Data  analysis 

The  data  were  first  assessed  using  a  three  way  repeated  measures 
ANOVA  with  the  following  factors:  duration  of  exposure  to  the  training 
diet  (restricted  vs.  ad  libitum),  the  large  meal  flavour  (anise  vs.  vanilla) 
and  cumulative  test  intervals  (30  min  vs.  4  h  vs.  24  h).  Follow  up 
limited  pair-wise  comparisons  between  groups  trained  under  similar 
conditions  of  exposure  to  the  training  diets  at  each  test  interval  were  also 
carried  out  using  independent  t  tests. 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

All  animals  completely  consumed  their  food  portions  on  all  training 
days.  Figure  3  outlines  the  results  obtained  during  the  three  cumulative 
intervals  of  the  extinction  choice  test  held  on  Day  9.  It  shows  that  the 
two  groups  which  had  received  vanilla  flavour  paired  with  the  larger 
meal  during  training  (i.e.,  Groups  R-V  and  Ad-V)  generally  had  higher 
preferences  for  anise  than  the  other  two  groups  (i.e.,  Groups  R-A  and 
Ad-A)  which  had  received  anise  paired  with  the  larger  meal  during 
training.  This  effect  was  particularly  marked  after  24  h  than  after  both 
the  30  min  and  4  h  cumulative  intervals  of  the  extinction  choice  test. 

The  repeated  measures  ANOVA  failed  to  reveal  any  significant  main 
effects  (all  ps  >  0.05).  However,  the  interaction  between  all  three  factors 
was  significant,  F(2,40)  =  5.82,  p  <  0.01.  Additional  between-group 
comparisons  also  revealed  a  highly  significant  difference  between 
Groups  Ad-A  and  Ad-V  after  24  h  of  testing  only,  /  (10,  one  tailed)  = 
5.07,  p  <  0.001  (see  Figure  3).  This  shows  that  the  rat  pups  were  clearly 
capable  of  learning  to  associate  a  taste  with  the  size  of  the  meal  under 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI 


17 


30  MIN 


4  HOURS 
TEST  INTERVAL 


24  HOURS 


GROUP  R-A  E]  GROUP  R-V  D  GROUP  Ad-A  ^  GROUP  Ad-V 


Figure  3.   Mean  preferences  (%)  for  anise  flavour  during  the  two-jar  extinction  choice 
tests  in  Experiment  2  (n  =  6).    Bars  represent  standard  errors. 


the  ad  libitum  feeding  conditions,  whereas  they  were  not  able  to  do  so 
under  restricted  feeding  conditions.  Even  more  interesting  was  the  fact 
that  flavour  preference  shifts  were  most  pronounced  after  24  h  of  testing 
than  earlier.  This  finding  suggests  that  the  flavour-meal  size 
conditioning  effects  observed  in  these  experiments  might  be  attributed 
to  satiety  rather  than  aversions,  because  if  the  latter  were  involved, 
flavour  preference  shifts  would  have  been  observed  throughout  the  test 
period.  In  particular,  they  would  have  commenced  at  the  beginning  of 
the  test  period  rather  than  later,  as  occurred  in  the  present  experiment. 
The  results  are  consistent  with  Booth's  (1972)  conditioned  satiety  ideas 
but  they  clearly  contradict  Hayward's  (1983)  findings.  It  is  important  to 
note  here  that  the  present  experiment  used  a  procedure  similar  to 
Hayward's  except  that  the  meal  sizes  and  one  flavour  were  different. 
However,  her  findings  generally  supported  a  conditioned  appetite 
hypothesis  whereby  only  young  rats  learned  to  prefer  a  taste  which  was 
previously  associated  with  larger  meal  sizes  or  more  calorific  diets. 


18  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

EXPERIMENT  4 

As  both  the  training  and  testing  procedures  employed  in  Experiment 
3  did  differ  somewhat  from  those  employed  in  both  earlier  experiments 
(i.e.,  Experiments  1  and  2),  it  is  not  only  difficult  but  also  rather  unfair 
to  make  direct  comparisons  between  their  respective  findings.  Clearly, 
the  best  way  to  achieve  such  a  goal  would  be  to  replicate  Experiment  3 
using  naive  adult  rats.  This  was  the  aim  of  Experiment  4.  According 
to  Hay  ward  (1983),  an  adult  rat  no  longer  attends  to  post-ingestional 
feedback  since  it  is  rarely  hungry  and  therefore  is  not  capable  of  learning 
conditioned  taste  preferences  based  on  differing  caloric  outcome. 
Therefore,  Experiment  4  investigated  whether  naive  adult  rats  could  be 
as  responsive  as  naive  young  rats  to  flavours  paired  with  differing 
caloric  outcome  when  novel  diets  provided  either  only  a  small  proportion 
or  most  of  the  rats'  daily  caloric  needs. 

METHOD 

Animals 

Twenty  four  naive  male  hooded  Lister  rats  81  -  126  days-old  and 
weighing  245  -  420  gm  at  the  start  of  the  experiment  were  used. 

Materials  and  Procedure 

The  materials  and  procedure  were  the  same  as  for  Experiment  3 
except  for  the  following  three  minor  changes:  both  Groups  R-A  and  R- 
V  were  allowed  25  gm  of  supplementary  solid  laboratory  chow  pellets 
at  17.00  hrs  each  day;  during  familiarisation,  both  Groups  Ad-A  and 
Ad-V  received  30  gm  of  unflavoured  and  novel  high  calorie  diet  instead 
of  20  gm  only  as  in  Experiment  3;  during  training,  both  Groups  Ad-A 
and  Ad-V  received  30  gm  of  the  novel  diet  with  no  supplements  on 
large  meal  days  and  15  gm  of  the  novel  diet  plus  another  15  gm 
supplement  of  solid  laboratory  chow  pellets  on  small  meal  days.  All  the 
changes  were  made  to  compensate  for  the  larger  size  of  animals  used  in 
the  present  experiment  compared  to  those  used  in  Experiment  3. 


RESULTS  AND  DISCUSSION 

All  animals  completely  consumed  their  food  portions  on  all  training 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI 


19 


30  MIN 


4  HOURS 
TEST  INTERVAL 


24  HOURS 


GROUP  R-A  E]  GROUP  R-V  DgROUP  Ad-A  ^  GROUP  Ad-V 


Figure  4.   Mean  preferences  (%)  for  anise  flavour  during  the  two-jar  extinction  choice 
test  in  Experiment  3  (n  =  6).    Bars  represent  standard  errors. 


days.  The  results  obtained  during  the  three  interval  of  the  extinction 
choice  test  held  on  Day  9  are  displayed  in  Figure  4.  They  show  that 
Group  Ad-V  generally  had  higher  preferences  for  anise  flavour  than 
Group  Ad-A,  whereas  no  such  differences  are  evident  between  Groups 
R-A  and  R-V. 

When  the  data  were  assessed  using  a  three  way  repeated  measures 
ANOVA  as  for  Experiment  3,  neither  the  main  effects  nor  interactions 
were  significant.  However,  additional  between-group  comparisons  of 
anise  preferences  indicated  that  Group  Ad-A  had  a  significantly  higher 
preference  than  Group  Ad-V  after  the  4  h  test  interval  only,  /  (10,  one 
tailed)  =  2.20,  p  <  0.05.  Thus,  these  mature  rats  were  also  able  to  learn 
about  flavour-meal  size  associations  under  ad  libitum  but  not  restricted 
feeding  conditions,  as  did  the  weanling  ones  in  Experiment  3.  It  is 
rather  difficult  to  explain  why  adult  rats  appeared  to  learn  conditioned 
satiety  only  when  flavour-meal  size  pairings  were  presented  on  an  ad 
libitum  basis  (30  gm  vs.  15  gm)  but  not  when  access  was  restricted  to 


20  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

much  smaller  amounts  (5  gm  vs.  1  gm).  Perhaps,  in  the  latter  condition 
adult  rats  simply  ignored  post-ingestional  feedback  of  the  small  snacks 
with  a  novel  flavour  and  concentrated  mostly  on  the  bigger 
supplementary  meals  from  which  most  of  their  daily  caloric  requirements 
were  derived.  Such  a  view  is  consistent  with  that  of  Bolles  et  al.  (1981) 
and  Hay  ward  (1983)  who  used  this  fact  to  argue  why  adult  rats  should 
fail  to  learn  to  associate  a  flavour  with  a  diet  that  provides  more  calories 
under  such  conditions.  Nevertheless,  the  fact  that,  like  weanling  rats, 
adults  can  acquire  flavour-meal  size  effects,  appears  to  contradict  these 
authors'  findings.  Another  interesting  aspect  of  the  present  results  is  that 
preference  shifts  in  those  groups  which  displayed  them  were  most 
evident  only  after  4  h  of  testing.  This  differs  from  Experiment  3  where 
preference  shifts  were  most  notable  in  the  groups  concerned  after  24  h 
of  testing.  Although  the  reasons  for  this  disparity  are  unclear  at  present, 
one  possibility  is  that  older  rats  may  experience  the  onset  of  satiety 
much  sooner  than  younger  ones.  Nevertheless,  this  issue  needs  to  be 
investigated  further.  Experiment  4,  therefore,  has  shown  that  adult  rats 
appear  to  learn  to  make  flavour-meal  size  associations  just  as  well  as 
weanling  rats  especially  when  the  diet  provides  for  most  of  the  animals 
daily  caloric  needs. 


GENERAL  DISCUSSION 

The  present  research  showed  that  both  weanling  and  mature  rats  are 
capable  of  learning  to  associate  a  flavour  with  the  size  of  a  meal.  In 
particular,  these  findings  demonstrated  flavour-meal  size  conditioning 
effects  whereby  an  animal  learns  to  prefer  a  flavour  not  associated  with 
a  larger  meal.  It  is  however  important  to  note  here  that,  as  Capaldi  and 
Myers  (1982)  and  Davidson-Codjoe  and  Holman  (1982)  have  pointed 
out,  the  preferences  demonstrated  in  experiments  of  this  sort  are  relative, 
rather  than  absolute.  The  data  really  do  not  distinguish  a  preference  for 
the  flavour  consumed  when  rats  received  the  small  meal  from  an 
aversion  for  the  flavour  consumed  when  they  received  the  large  meal. 
Furthermore,  as  Capaldi  et  al.  (1983)  also  pointed  out,  the  word 
"preference"  is  usually  intended  as  a  neutral  term  accommodating  either 
a  learned  aversion  for  the  flavour  associated  with  the  large  meal,  learned 
liking  for  the  flavour  associated  with  the  small  meal,  or  both. 

As  the  present  results  are  consistent  with  Booth's  (1972)  and  Booth 
and  Davis'  (1973)  findings,  a  phenomenon  very  similar  to  Booth's 
conditioned  satiety  may  be  implicated.  Conditioned  satiety  involves  an 
increment  in  the  size  of  the  feeding  bout  on  the  dilute  nutrient  (or,  small 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  21 

meal,  few  calories,  etc.)  more  than  on  the  more  concentrated  nutrient  (or 
large  meal,  many  calories,  etc.)  after  several  pairs  of  presentations.  This 
takes  place  in  order  to  compensate  for  calories  depending  on  the  nature 
of  the  concentrated  diet.  Booth  suggested  that  such  conditioning  of 
satiety  may  be  important  in  the  control  of  food  intake  only  under 
conditions  which  are  put  on  the  timing  of  meals,  that  is,  under  high  food 
deprivation  schedules.  Perhaps,  different  mechanisms  are  involved  under 
normal  ad  libitum  feeding  conditions.  This  might  explain  why  stronger 
evidence  of  conditioned  satiety  learning  was  obtained  under  restricted 
feeding  conditions  in  both  Experiments  1  and  2  in  the  present  study  than 
was  the  case  under  ad  libitum  feeding  conditions  in  both  Experiments  3 
and  4.  Nevertheless,  the  present  findings  clearly  demonstrated  the 
existence  of  an  acquired  oral  and/or  olfactory  sensory  control  of  the 
satiation  process  just  as  Booth's  conditioned  satiety  study  did.  The 
present  findings  are  also  in  agreement  with  those  of  Le  Magnen  (1985) 
and  Deutsch  (1982). 

The  present  findings  suggest  that  oral  qualities  of  a  familiar  food 
may  enable  a  mammal  to  react  in  anticipation  of  that  food's  caloric  value 
or  the  duration  of  its  satiating  effect.  According  to  such  a  view,  animals 
stop  eating  at  an  appropriate  stage  even  though  absorption  has  barely 
started  or  possibly  has  not  even  started  at  all  as  in  the  case  of  a  rapid 
feeder  such  as  a  dog.  However,  our  findings  clearly  contradict 
Hay  ward's  (1983)  findings  which  generally  demonstrated  conditioned 
appetite  in  weanling  rats  but  not  in  adults.  Apart  from  strain  differences, 
it  is  unclear  at  present  why  our  results  contradict  Hayward's. 

One  hypothesis  which  explains  the  present  findings  is  that  the 
flavour  tracking  may  be  due  to  an  acquired  difference  in  the 
development  of  feeding  inhibition  during  meals.  This  is  because  there 
were  no  differences  in  basal  acceptability  since  both  meals  consisted  of 
the  same  diet,  except  for  flavours  which  were  added  to  them.  This  view 
is  consistent  with  that  of  Booth  (1972).  The  higher  preference  for  the 
flavour  which  had  been  paired  with  a  small  meal  demonstrated  in  the 
present  study  could  be  attributed  to  a  difference  in  the  speed  of  onset  of 
the  suppression  of  intake  in  the  later  stages  of  the  meal.  This  might 
suggest  that  an  acquired  oral  and/or  olfactory  factor  contributed  to  the 
development  of  the  satiety  which  ended  the  meal  to  which  the  flavour 
previously  associated  with  the  large  meal  was  added.  When  faced  with 
a  choice  between  flavour  cues  previously  associated  with  large  and  small 
meals  during  conditioning,  a  satiety  signal  switched  off  eating 
immediately  after  the  animal  had  perceived  that  the  flavour  was 
previously  correlated  with  a  large  meal  which  was  too  satiating. 
However,  there  was  no  such  satiety  signal  forthcoming  from  flavour 


22  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

correlated  with  less  satiating  small  meals  and  consequently,  rats 
consumed  significantly  more  food  with  a  flavour  which  was  previously 
associated  with  a  small,  less  satiating  meal.  Hence,  satiety  had  no 
positive  reinforcing  effects  and  might  have  been  aversive.  As  was 
mentioned  earlier,  such  a  conclusion  is  consistent  with  that  of  Van  Vort 
and  Smith's  (1983)  who  found  that  satiety  had  no  positive  reinforcing 
effects  on  flavour  preferences  at  all  whilst  using  sham  feeding  techniques 
and  a  similar  flavour  tracking  procedure. 

An  alternative  explanation  is  that  when  rats  were  exposed  to  flavours 
correlated  with  large  meals,  they  developed  slightly  stronger  aversions 
to  them  than  when  exposed  to  flavours  correlated  with  small  meals. 
However,  since  Experiments  2-4  controlled  for  the  amount  of  flavour 
exposure,  this  hypothesis  is  highly  improbable.  Furthermore,  the  mere 
fact  that  all  groups  in  Experiment  1  ate  in  excess  of  60%  vanilla- 
flavoured  food  during  choice  tests  further  weakens  this  argument. 

The  conditioning  of  flavour  preferences  based  on  meal  size 
demonstrated  in  the  present  research  does  not  in  any  way  contradict  Le 
Magnen's  (1969)  findings  of  conditioned  appetite  at  all.  Conditioned 
discriminative  (also  referred  to  as  selective  or  differential)  appetite 
induced  during  conditioning  causes  a  rat  to  eat  more  of  the  reinforced, 
and  hence  beneficial  food,  and  less  of  the  non  reinforced  food  in  the 
final  choice  test.  As  Bolles  et  al.  (1981)  pointed  out,  conditioned  satiety 
and  conditioned  appetite  merely  represent  complementary  aspects  of 
intake  regulation.  Whereas  the  phenomenon  proposed  by  Le  Magnen 
(which  Bolles  et  al.  later  failed  to  demonstrate)  was  basically  concerned 
with  food  selection  through  learning  about  the  caloric  potential  of 
different  foods,  the  phenomenon  demonstrated  in  the  present  study  more 
likely  operates  after  the  selection  of  the  diet  offering  optimum  caloric 
consequences  has  been  completed.  Hence,  the  animal  learns  to  eat  less 
of  that  diet  on  a  day-after-day  basis  under  conditions  in  which 
constraints  are  put  on  the  timing  of  meals. 

According  to  Capaldi  et  al.  (1987),  shifts  in  preferences  such  as 
those  demonstrated  in  the  present  study  can  be  viewed  within  either  a 
classical  conditioning  or  an  instrumental  learning  paradigm.  In  a 
classical  conditioning  framework,  the  flavours  are  the  conditioned  stimuli 
(CSs)  and  they  are  being  associated  with  some  unconditioned  stimulus 
(US)  that  is  produced  by  consumption  of  meals  of  different  sizes  made 
from  the  same  diet.  In  an  instrumental  learning  paradigm,  the  flavours 
are  the  discriminatory  stimuli  (SDs)  signalling  the  reinforcement 
produced  by  the  instrumental  response  of  consuming  meals  of  different 
sizes.  The  important  question  therefore  concerns  what  is  the  US  or 
reinforcer  in  either  paradigm. 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  23 

One  of  the  best  candidates  for  the  US  or  reinforcer  in  the  type  of 
learning  demonstrated  in  the  present  study  is  the  post-ingestive 
consequences  of  ingesting  meals  such  as  calories.  Supporting  evidence 
for  flavour-calorie  learning  in  rats  has  been  reported  elsewhere  (e.g., 
Capaldi  et  al.,  1987;  Deems  et  al.,  1986;  Hayward,  1983;  Holman,  1975; 
Mehiel  and  Bolles,  1984;  Sherman,  Hickis,  Rice,  Rusiniak  &  Garcia, 
1983).  Another  possible  candidate  for  the  US  or  reinforcer  is  some  oral 
stimulus  such  as  flavour  (or  taste).  However,  this  is  very  unlikely 
because  the  different  sized  meals  in  each  experiment  in  the  present  study 
consisted  of  exactly  the  same  diet,  which  meant  that  the  flavour  of  the 
food  contained  in  the  two  meals  was  identical.  Thus,  the  flavour-meal 
size  conditioning  effects  demonstrated  in  the  present  research  were  most 
probably  the  result  of  flavour-calorie  rather  than  flavour-flavour 
associations. 

Finally,  it  must  be  noted  here  that  although  only  four  experiments 
are  reported  in  the  present  paper,  six  additional  experiments  were  also 
carried  out  which  consistently  failed  to  yield  any  significant  effects. 
However,  preferences  were  mainly  in  the  direction  which  is  predicted  by 
the  conditioned  satiety  hypothesis,  that  is,  the  small  meal  flavour  was 
relatively  more  preferred.  These  findings  also  suggest  that  the 
flavour-meal  size  conditioning  effect  definitely  occurs  but  we  have  not 
yet  been  able  to  identify  all  the  conditions  which  enable  consistently 
reliable  effects. 

In  conclusion,  both  mature  and  weanling  rats  appear  to  be  capable 
of  learning  about  flavour-meal  size  associations.  In  particular,  they 
acquired  conditioned  satiety,  albeit  very  weakly,  rather  than  conditioned 
appetite  as  previously  demonstrated  by  both  Bolles  et  al.  (1981)  and 
Hayward  (1983)  in  their  similar  studies.  These  conditioned  satiety 
effects,  which  are  most  probably  due  to  flavour-calorie  rather  than 
flavour-flavour  (or  tastes)  associations,  are  also  distinctly  elusive. 


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 

This  research  was  carried  out  in  the  Laboratory  of  Experimental 
Psychology  at  the  University  of  Sussex,  Brighton,  England,  U.K.,  as  part 
of  a  D.  Phil,  project.  It  was  partially  financed  by  a  U.K.  Medical 
Research  grant  to  Dr.  M.J.  Burton  and  also  by  a  University  of  Zambia 
Staff  Development  Fellowship  to  the  author.  The  author  wishes  to 
acknowledge,  with  deep  gratitude,  the  immense  help  received  during  the 
study  from  Drs.  Burton  and  R.  A.  Boakes,  both  of  the  Laboratory  of 
Experimental  Psychology  at  Sussex  University  at  the  time,  who  jointly 


24  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

served  as  supervisors  for  this  project.  Their  constant  encouragement 
throughout  his  sojourn  at  Sussex  University  is  especially  appreciated. 
The  author  would  also  like  to  thank  Sara  Hill,  Tracey  Gummet, 
Annabele  Poate  and  Mark  Allen  for  helping  carry  out  Experiment  2  as 
part  of  their  undergraduate  research  project  under  his  supervision  and 
also  Dr  Christopher  J.  Rainey  for  his  assistance  with  running  some  of  the 
experiments.  Finally,  the  comments  and  suggestions  of  two  anonymous 
reviewers  are  greatly  appreciated. 


REFERENCES 

Boakes,  R.A.,  &  Lubart,  T.  (1988).  Enhanced  preference  for  a  flavour  following  reversed 

flavour-glucose  pairing.  Quarterly  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology,  B40,  49-62. 
Boakes,  R.A.,  Rossi-Arnaud,  C,  &  Garcia-Hoz,  V.   (1987).  Early  experience  and 

reinforcer  quality  in  delayed  flavour- food  learning  in  the  rat.  Appetite,  9,  191-206. 
Bolles,  R.C.  (1983).  A  "mixed"  model  of  taste  preference.  In  R.L.  Mellgren  (Ed.), 

Advances    in    Psychology    (Vol.    3):    Animal    cognition    and   animal    behavior. 

Amsterdam:  North-Holland. 
Bolles,  R.C,  Hayward,  L.,  &  Crandall,  C  (1981).  Conditioned  taste  preferences  based 

on    caloric    density.    Journal   of  Experimental    Psychology:    Animal   Behavior 

Processes,  7,  59-69. 
Booth.   D.A.   (1972).   Conditioned   satiety   in   the  rat.   Journal  of  Comparative  and 

Physiological  Psychology,  81,  457-47 1 . 
Booth,    D.A.    (1985).    Food-conditioned    eating    preferences    and    aversions    with 

interoceptive  elements:  Conditioned  appetites  and  satieties.  In  N.S.  Braveman  &  P. 

Bronstein  (Eds.),  Experimental  assessments  and  clinical  applications  of  conditioned 

food  aversions.  Annals  of  the   N.Y.  Academy  of  Science,  443,  22-41. 
Booth,  D.A.,  &  Davis,  J.D.  (1973).  Gastrointestional  factors  in  the  acquisition  of  oral 

sensory  control  of  satiation.  Physiology  and  Behavior,  11,  23-29. 
Booth,  D.A.,  &  Mather,  P.  (1978).  Prototype  model  of  human  feeding  growth  and 

obesity.  In  D.A.  Booth  (Ed.),  Hunger  models.  New  York:  Academic  Press. 
Booth,  D.A.,  Lee,  M.,  &  Aleavey,  C.  (1976).  Acquired  sensory  control  of  satiation  in 

man.  British  Journal  of  Psychology,  67,  137-147. 
Booth,   D.A.,   Mather,   P.   &   Fuller,   J.   (1982).   Starch   content   of  ordinary   foods 

associatively  conditions  human  appetite  and  satiation,  indexed  by  intake  and  eating 

pleasantness  of  starch-paired  flavours.  Appetite,  3,  16-184. 
Booth,  D.A.,  Stoloff,  R.,  &  Nicholls,  J.  (1974).  Dietary  flavour  acceptance  in  infant  rats 

established  by  association  with  effects  of  nutrient  composition.   Physiological 

Psychology,  2,  313-319. 
Capaldi,  E.D.,  &  Myers,  D.E.  (1982).  Taste  preferences  as  a  function  of  food  deprivation 

during  original  taste  exposure.  Animal  Learning  and  Behavior,  10,  21 1-219. 
Capaldi,  E.D.,  Campbell,  D.H.,  Sheffer,  J.D.,  &  Bradford,  J. P.  (1987).  Conditioned 

flavour  preferences  based  on  delayed  caloric  consequences.  Journal  of  Experimental 

Psychology:  Animal  Behavior  Processes,  13,  150-155. 
Capaldi,  E.D.,  Myers,  D.E.,  Campbell,  D.H.,  &  Sheffer,  J.D.  (1983).  Conditioned  flavour 

preferences   based   on   hunger   level   during   original   flavour  exposure.   Animal 


LEICKNESS  C.  SIMBAYI  25 

Learning  and  Behavior,  J],  107-115. 
Capretta,  P.J.  (1961).  An  experimental  modification  of  food  preferences  in  chickens. 

Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology,  54,  238-242. 
Crawford,  D.,  &  Baker,  T.B.  (1982).  Alcohol  dependence  and  taste-mediated  learning 

in  the  rat.  Pharmacology,  Biochemistry  and   Behavior,  16,  253-261. 
Davidson-Codjoe,  M.,  &  Holman,  E.W.  (1982).  The  effect  of  non-nutritive  satiation  on 

the  learning  of  a  flavour  preference  by  rats.  Animal  Learning  and  Behavior,  10, 

220-222. 
Deems,  D.A.,  Getting,  R.L.,  Sherman,  J.E.,  &  Garcia,  J.  (1986).  Hungry,  but  not  thirsty, 

rats  prefer  flavours  paired  with  ethanol.  Physiology  and  Behavior,  36,  141-144. 
Deutsch,  J. A.  (1982).  Controversies  in  food  intake.  In  B.C.  Hoebel  &  D.  Novin  (Eds.), 

The  neural  basis  of  feeding  and  reward.  Brunswick,  Maine,  U.S.A.:  Haer  Institute 

of  Electrophysiological  Research. 
Deutsch,  J.A.  (1983).  Dietary  control  and  the  stomach.  In  G.A.  Kerkut,  &  J.W.  Phillips 

(Eds.),  Progress  in  neurobiology.  (Vol.  20)  Oxford:  Pergamon  Press. 
Deutsch,  J.A.  (1987).  Signals  determining  meal  size.  In  R.A.  Boakes,  D.A.  Popplewell, 

&  M.J.  Burton  (Eds.),  Eating  habits.  Chichester:  John  Wiley. 
Deutsch,  J.A.,   &  Gonzalez,   M.F.   (1980).   Gastric   nutrient  content   signals   satiety. 

Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology,  30,  113-116. 
Deutsch,  J. A.,  &  Walton,  N.Y.  (1977).  A  rat  alcoholism  model  in  a  free  choice  situation. 

Behavioral  Biology,  19,  349-360 
Deutsch,  J.A.,  Gonzalez,  M.F.,  &  Young,  M.G.  (1980).  Two  factors  control  meal  size. 

Brain  Research  Bulletin,  5(4),  55-58. 
Fanselow,  M.S.,  &  Berk,  J.  (1982).  Flavour-flavour  associations  induce  hedonic  shifts 

in  taste  preferences.  Animal  Learning  and  Behavior,  10,  223-228. 
Hay  ward,  L.  (1983).  The  role  and  postingestional  cues  in  the  conditioning  of  taste 

preferences  based  on  differing  caloric  density  and  caloric  outcome  in  weanling  and 

mature  rats.  Animal  Learning  and  Behavior,  11,  325-331. 
Hogan,  J.A.  (1977).  The  ontogeny  of  food  preferences  in  chicks  and  other  animals.  In 

L.M.  Barker,  M.R.  Best.,  &  M.  Domjan  (Eds.),  Learning  mechanisms  in  food 

selection.  Waco,  Texas:  Baylor  University  Press. 
Hogan,  J.A.  (1980).  Homeostasis  and  behavior.  In  F.M.  Toates,  &  R.R.  Halliday  (Eds.), 

Analysis  of  motivational  processes.  London:  Academic  Press. 
Holman,  E.W.  (1975).  Immediate  and  delayed  reinforcers  for  flavour  preferences  in  rats. 

Learning  and  Motivation,  6,  9 1  - 1 00. 
Le  Magnen,  J.  (1969).  Peripheral  and  systemic  actions  of  food  in  the  caloric  regulation 

of  intake.  Annals  of  the  N.Y.  Academy  of  Science,  57,  1 126-1 156. 
Le  Magnen,  J.  (1971).  Advances  in  studies  on  the  physiological  control  and  regulation 

of  food  intake.  In  E.  Steller,  &  J.M.  Spraque  (Eds.),  Progress  in  physiological 

psychology  (Vol.  4).  New  York:  Academic  Press. 
Le  Magnen,  J.  (1985).  Hunger.  Cambridge,  U.K.:  Cambridge  University  Press. 
Le  Magnen,  J.  (1987).  Palatability:  Concept,  terminology  and  mechanisms.  In  R.A. 

Boakes,  D.A.  Popplewell,  &  M.J.  Burton  (Eds.),  Eating  habits.  Chichester:  John 

Wiley. 
Le    Magnen,    J.,    Marfaing-Jallat,    P.,    &    Miceli,    D.    (1980).     A    bioassay    of 

ethanol-dependence    in    rats.    Pharmacology,    Biochemistry   and   Behavior,    12, 

707-709. 
Logue,  A.W.  (1979).  Taste  aversion  and  the  generality  of  laws  of  learning.  Psychological 

Bulletin,  86,  276-296. 


26  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

Marfaing-Jallat,   P.,   &   Le   Magnen,   J.   (1979).   Ethanol-induced   taste   aversion   in 

ethanol-dependent  and  normal  rats.  Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology,  26,  106-114. 
Mehiel,  R.,  &  Bolles,  R.C.   (1984).  Learned  flavour  preferences  based  on  caloric 

outcome.  Animal  Learning  and  Behavior,  12,  421-427. 
Revusky,  S.H.  (1967).  Hunger  level  during  food  consumption:  Effects  on  subsequent 

preference.  Psychonomic  Science,  7,  109-110. 
Revusky,  S.H.  (1968).  Effects  of  thirst  level  during  consumption  of  flavoured  water  on 

subsequent  preference.  Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology,  66, 

111-119. 
Revusky,  S.H.  (1974).  Retention  of  learned  increase  in  the  preference  for  flavoured 

solution.  Behavioral  Biology,  11,  121-1254. 
Riley,  A.L.,  &  Tuck,  D.L.  (1985).  Conditioned  taste  aversions:  A  bibliography.  In  N.S. 

Braveman,    &    P.    Bronstein    (Eds.),    Experimental    assessments    and    clinical 

applications  of  conditioned  food  aversions.  Annals  of  the  N.Y.  Academy  of  Science, 

443,  381-437. 
Rozin,  P.  (1977).  The  significance  of  learning  mechanisms  in  food  selection:  Some 

biology,  psychology  and  sociology  of  science.  In  L.M.  Barker,  M.R.  Best,  and  M. 

Domjan  (Eds.),  Learning  mechanisms  in  food  selection.   Waco,  Texas:   Baylot 

University  Press. 
Sherman,  J.E.,  Hickis,  C.F.,  Rice,  A.G.,  Rusianik,  K.W.,  &  Garcia,  J.  (1983).  Preferences 

and  aversions  for  stimuli  paired  with  ethanol  in  hungry  rats.  Animal  Learning  and 

Behavior,  77,  101-106. 
Simbayi,  L.C.  (1987).  Immediate  and  delayed  flavour-calorie  learning.  Can  rats  do  it? 

International  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology,  1,    58-77. 
Simbayi,  L.C,  Boakes,  R.A.,  &  Burton,  M.J  (1986).  Can  rats  learn  to  associate  a  flavour 

with  the  delayed  delivery  of  food?  Appetite,  7,  41-53. 
Smith,  G.P.,  &  Gibbs,  J.  (1979).  Postprandial  satiety.  In  J.M.  Sprague,  &  A.N.  Epstein 

(Eds.),  Progress  in  psychobiology  and  physiological  psychology  (vol.  8).  New  York: 

Academic  Press. 
Van  Vort,  W.,  &  Smith,  G.P.  (1983).  The  relationships  between  the  positive  reinforcing 

and  satiating  effects  of  a  meal  in  the  rat.  Physiology  and  Behavior,  30,  279-284. 


Internationa!  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology,  Vol.  7,  No.  1,  1994 


LIGHT  MEDIATION  OF  CIRCADIAN 
PREDATORY  BEHAVIOR  IN  THE  YOUNG 

ALLIGATOR 

Jack  A.  Palmer 

Linda  K.  Palmer 

Northeast  Louisiana  University 


ABSTRACT:  Minnow  predation  by  10  young  American  alligators  (Alligator 
mississippiensis)  was  systematically  measured  during  four  daily  time  periods  under 
four  different  conditions  of  lighting  in  order  to  investigate  a  circadian  rhythm  of 
predatory  behavior.  The  four  daily  time  periods  were  night  (1:00  a.m. -7.00  a.m.), 
morning  (7.00  a.m.- 1:00  p.m.),  afternoon  (1:00  p.m.-7:00  p.m.),  and  evening  (7:00 
p.m. -1:00  a.m.).  Each  of  the  following  lighting  conditions  had  a  duration  of  4  weeks: 
continuous  complete  darkness  (DD);  continuous  artificial  illumination  (LL);  naturally 
varying  Ught-dark  conditions  (natural  LD);  and  reversed  light-dark  conditions  with 
artificial  lights  on  at  sunset  and  off  at  sunrise  (reversed  LD).  Predatory  behavior  (i.e., 
the  number  of  prey  fish  consumed  wholly  or  partially  during  each  test  session)  varied 
significantly  as  a  function  of  the  interaction  between  time  period  and  lighting 
condition.  Under  natural  LD,  the  mean  number  of  prey  killed  during  night  sessions 
was  significantly  higher  than  either  morning  or  afternoon  sessions.  Under  reversed 
LD,  the  pattern  of  predation  reversed  from  that  exhibited  under  normal  Ughting,  with 
both  morning  and  afternoon  predation  significantly  higher  than  either  evening  or 
night.  Under  conditions  of  continuous  illumination  (LL  and  DD)  the  natural  LD 
circadian  pattern  persisted  for  over  1  week  with  significantly  higher  predation  rates 
during  the  night  periods  as  compared  to  the  morning  and  afternoon  periods.  The 
gradual  transition  of  predation  pattern  in  response  to  manipulations  of  the  light-dark 
cycle  suggests  that  the  circadian  rhythm  of  alligator  predadon  is  dependent  upon 
light-dark  variation  for  entrainment. 


INTRODUCTION 

The  adult  American  alligator  {Alligator  mississippiensis)  has  been 
described  as  an  opportunistic  scavenger  as  well  as  an  active  predator 


Address    correspondence    to    Jack    A.    Palmer,    Department    of    Psychology, 
Northeast  Louisiana  University,  Monroe,  LA  71209,  USA. 

©  1994  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology  27 


28  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

(Mcllhenny,  1976;  Pooley,  1989;  Weldon,  Swenson,  Olson,  & 
Brinkmeier,  1990).  Hatchling  alligators  eat  insects,  small  crustaceans, 
and  small  fish  (Coulson  &  Hernandez,  1983),  whereas  adult  alligators 
prey  upon  frogs,  snakes,  birds,  muskrats,  nutria,  and  larger  mammals 
(Mcllhenny,  1976;  Pooley,  1989;  Ross  &  Magnusson,  1989;  Scott  & 
Weldon,  1990).  The  alligator  is  generally  considered  a  nocturnal 
species  with  most  of  its  feeding  behavior  and  general  activity  taking 
place  during  the  night  (Lang,  1976;  Mcllhenny,  1976;  Murphy,  1981; 
Pooley,  1989).  Alligator  predatory  behavior  appears  to  follow  a 
circadian  rhythm,  but  this  has  not  been  systematically  investigated  and 
reported. 

In  almost  all  vertebrates,  endogenous  circadian  rhythms  regulate 
temporally  certain  types  of  behavior  (e.g.,  sleep)  and  bodily  functions 
(e.g.,  hormone  secretions,  Aschoff,  1989).  The  rhythm  itself  is  often 
entrained  or  synchronized  by  environmental  cues  such  as  daily  cycles 
of  light-dark  and  temperature  change  (Aschoff,  1989;  Griffiths,  1986). 
These  environmental  cues  that  set  the  timing  of  circadian  behavior  are 
referred  to  as  zeitgebers  (Aschoff,  1989).  Aschoff  reports  that  the 
most  powerful  of  all  zeitgebers  is  a  light-dark  cycle.  In  the  absence 
of  zeitgebers  (e.g.,  an  environment  of  continuous  darkness),  circadian 
rhythms  are  free-running  and  self-sustaining.  The  free-running 
rhythm  may  cycle  every  24  hours,  be  slightly  longer,  or  slightly 
shorter. 

In  lower  vertebrates,  light  mediates  circadian  rhythm  via  the 
photosensitive  pineal  gland.  Although  alligators  lack  pineal  glands, 
studies  have  indicated  that  some  circadian  rhythms  in  alligators  are 
controlled  or  mediated  by  light  cycles.  Circadian  rhythm  of  young 
alligator  motor  activity  was  documented  by  Kavaliers  and  Ralph 
(1980).  Lang  (1976)  found  that  movement  between  land  and  water  in 
juvenile  alligators  varied  as  a  function  of  circadian  rhythm  cued  by 
light-dark  cycles.  A  study  by  Murphy  (1981)  indicated  the  existence 
of  an  endogenous  clock  synchronized  by  the  circadian  light-dark  cycle 
which  enabled  young  alligators  to  use  solar  compass  orientation. 
Kavaliers  (1980)  demonstrated  that  extraretinal  responses  to  light 
pulses  varied  according  to  circadian  phase.  Moreover,  photoperiod 
manipulation  indicated  exogenous  entrainment  of  the  extraretinal 
responses  by  light-dark  cycles.  In  light  of  the  alligators'  lack  of  a 
pineal  gland,  Kavaliers  and  Ralph  (1981)  argued  that  overall 
organization  of  circadian  rhythm  and  period  length  depends  on  a 
complex  interaction  of  retinal  inputs,  extraretinal  inputs,  and  other 
neural  areas. 

This  study  sought  to  determine  if  alligator  predation  was  subject 


JACK  PALMER  AND  LINDA  PALMER 


29 


to  circadian  rhymicity.  Minnow  predation  (i.e.,  number  of  minnows 
wholly  or  partially  consumed)  by  young  alligators  was  systematically 
measured  during  four  daily  time  periods  under  different  conditions  of 
lighting.  It  was  hypothesized  that  predation  rates  would  peak  during 
the  dark  periods  of  24-hour  natural  and  reversed  light-dark  cycles,  and 
that  an  endogenous  circadian  rhythm  would  persist  under  continuous 
illumination.  It  was  predicated  that  the  circadian  fluctuation  could  be 
altered  by  manipulating  the  light-dark  cycle,  thus  supporting 
dependence  upon  light-dark  alternation  as  a  zeitgeber.  A  gradual 
transition  of  predation  pattern  in  response  to  light-dark  cycle 
manipulations  would  indicate  entrainment  by  a  light-dark  zeitgeber. 

TABLE  1 

Subject  Size  After  the  Study  Compared  with  Number  of  Prey 

Captured  During  6-Hr  Sessions 


Subject  Size 

Number  of  Prey 

Captured 
Range 

Mean 

Subject 

Length 

Weight 

SD 

1 

46.0 

304.0 

0-9 

1.89 

2.28 

2 

46.5 

285.5 

0-10 

2.77 

2.94 

3 

42.5 

215.0 

0-10 

2.57 

2.97 

4 

42.5 

203.5 

0-10 

2.36 

2.78 

5 

41.9 

208.5 

0-10 

2.28 

2.84 

6 

38.5 

176.5 

0-9 

2.16 

2.37 

7 

39.0 

164.0 

0-6 

1.59 

1.84 

8 

44.0 

228.5 

0-10 

2.71 

2.94 

9 

37.0 

148.5 

0-5 

1.65 

1.73 

10 

38.0 

151.5 

0-10 

2.08 

2.48 

METHOD 
Subjects 


Ten  1990  hatchling  alligators  {Alligator  mississippiensis),  27  to  29 
cm  in  length  and  weighing  56  to  80  gm,  were  obtained  from  the 
Rockefeller  Wildlife   Refuge,   Grand  Chenier,   Louisiana   when   they 


30  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

were  approximately  1  month  of  age.  (Age  is  approximate  because 
exact  date  of  hatching  is  not  known).  Testing  began  when  subjects 
were  approximately  6  months  old  and  continued  until  subjects  were 
approximately  14  months  old.  At  the  completion  of  testing,  subjects 
ranged  from  37  to  46  cm  in  length  and  148.5  to  304  gm  in  weight 
(Table  1).  Individual  subjects  were  identified  by  removing  a  specific 
tail  scute. 

Maintenance 

When  not  being  tested,  all  10  alligators  were  maintained  in  a 
home  pool  measuring  45.72  cm  deep  and  2.44  m  in  diameter.  Ten 
cm  of  water  covered  the  bottom,  and  a  60.96  cm  diameter  gravel- 
surfaced  concrete  "island"  was  situated  near  the  center  of  the  pool. 
The  pool  was  retained  in  a  heated-  enclosed  garage  with  windows 
facing  north  and  east.  Air  temperature  ranged  from  21  to  28°C  with 
an  average  of  25°C.  Water  temperature  ranged  from  20  to  27°C  with 
an  average  of  24°C.  The  pool  was  drained  and  filled  with  fresh  water 
as  necessary  (usually  once  each  week). 

Food  included  two  species  of  live  minnows  (Notropis  cornutus 
and  Cyprinus  Idas  idus)  and  Burris  Alligator  Feed.  Alligators  were 
permitted  to  obtain  live  prey  (minnows)  beginning  when  they  were  2 
months  old.  Once  testing  began,  live  prey  was  available  only  in  the 
testing  arenas  during  test  sessions.  Burris  Alligator  Feed  was 
provided  ad  libitum  in  the  home  pool. 

Live  minnows  were  obtained  as  needed  (about  every  2  weeks) 
from  local  bait  shops  and  fish  supply  outlets.  They  were  maintained 
in  a  plastic  barrel,  55.88  cm  in  diameter  and  71.12  cm  high,  filled 
with  water.  Aeration  was  furnished  by  an  electric  Second  Nature 
Whisper  400  aquarium  pump.  Water  and  air  temperature  were  the 
same  as  that  for  the  alligators.  The  minnows  were  fed  an  adequate 
amount  of  Wardley  Tropical  Flakes  fish  food  every  evening. 

Apparatus 

Each  test  arena  consisted  of  a  plexiglass  tray  45.72  cm  x  25.4  cm 
X  12.7  cm  high  with  a  grill-type  metal  lid  that  allowed  air  circulation 
but  prevented  escape.  Each  arena  was  filled  with  7.62  cm  of  water. 
The  test  arenas  were  located  in  the  same  room  as  the  alligator  home 
pool.   Water  temperature  was  the  same  as  that  in  the  home  pool. 


JACK  PALMER  AND  LINDA  PALMER  31 

Procedure 

All  subjects  were  tested  under  all  conditions.  A  two-factor  (4  x 
4)  within-subjects  experimental  design  was  used,  with  one  factor 
(daily  time  period)  nested  within  the  other  factor  (lighting  condition). 

The  four  lighting  conditions  were  continuous  complete  darkness 
(DD),  continuous  artificial  illumination  (LL),  naturally  varying  light- 
dark  (natural  LD),  and  reversed  light-dark  with  artificial  light  on  at 
sunset  and  off  at  sunrise  (reversed  LD).  Each  lighting  condition  was 
maintained  for  4  weeks,  and  each  followed  a  2-week  period  of  natural 
lighting  to  control  for  carryover  effects.  (Kavaliers  and  Ralph  [1980] 
demonstrated  that,  following  light  manipulation,  10  days  of  natural 
light  were  sufficient  to  restore  normal  circadian  rhythmicity  of 
alligator  motor  activity). 

Artificial  illumination  was  provided  by  a  Power-Glo  40-watt  full 
spectrum  fluorescent  lamp.  When  necessary  to  shut  out  natural  light, 
the  windows  were  covered  with  black  shades  taped  tightly  against  the 
walls  so  that  no  light  could  enter. 

A  total  of  640  predation  test  sessions  of  6-hour  duration  were 
conducted  (160  test  sessions  per  lighting  condition).  Predation  test 
sessions  were  alternated  randomly  among  four  time  periods:  night 
(1:00  a.m.-7:00  a.m.),  morning  (7:00  a.m.- 1:00  p.m.),  afternoon  (1:00 
p.m. -7:00  p.m.),  and  evening  (7:00  p.m.- 1:00  a.m.).  No  time  period 
was  repeated  until  the  other  three  had  been  used.  All  four  time 
periods  were  tested  each  week. 

Because  movement  between  land  and  water  has  been 
demonstrated  to  follow  a  circadian  pattern  itself  (Lang,  1976), 
alligators  and  prey  animals  were  placed  in  test  arenas  to  control  for 
this  possible  confound.  Each  test  session  was  conducted  as  follows: 
ten  minnows,  five  specimens  each  of  Notropis  cornutus  and  Cyprinus 
idus  idus,  were  moved  with  a  net  by  the  investigator  from  their  home 
barrel  to  a  test  arena  at  the  beginning  of  the  designated  6-hr  time 
period.  An  alligator  was  then  carried  by  the  investigator  by  hand 
from  its  home  pool  to  the  test  arena.  A  grill-type  metal  lid  was  then 
placed  on  the  test  arena.  During  the  6-hr  test  session,  the  animals 
were  not  disturbed.  At  the  end  of  the  session,  the  number  of  prey 
killed  (i.e.,  number  of  minnows  missing  or  partially  consumed)  was 
recorded.  The  alligator  and  remaining  minnows  (if  any)  were  then 
returned  to  their  respective  homes.  The  water  in  the  test  arena  was 
emptied  and  replaced  with  fresh  water  after  each  test  session. 


32 
RESULTS 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


The  dependent  variable,  number  of  minnows  consumed  wholly  or 
partially,  was  analyzed  with  a  repeated  measures  analysis  of  variance 
(ANOVA),  in  which  time  period  and  lighting  condition  were 
independent  variables  (both  repeating)  with  time  periods  nested  within 
lighting  conditions.  Predatory  behavior  varied  significantly  as  a 
function  of  the  interaction  between  time  period  and  lighting  condition, 
F(9,270)  =  15.79,  /?<.001.  Mean  prey  killed  for  each  time  period 
within  each  lighting  condition  are  reported  in  Table  2. 

TABLE  2 

Means  and  Standard  Deviations  of  Prey  Killed  During  Time 

Periods  Under  Different  Lighting  Conditions 


Time  Periods 

Lighting  Conditions 

Night 

Morning 

Afternoon 

Evening 

Continuous  Dark 

Mean 

0.393 

0.260 

0.065 

0.203 

SD 

(0.507) 

(0.404) 

(0.227) 

(0.377) 

Continuous  Light 

Mean 

2.775 

2.975 

2.800 

2.350 

SD 

(2.769) 

(3.109) 

(2.757) 

(2.107) 

Natural  Light 

Mean 

2.700 

1.300 

1.250 

2.075 

SD 

(2.221) 

(1.488) 

(1.256) 

(1.542) 

Light  Reversal 

Mean 

2.600 

4.500 

4.600 

1.750 

SD 

(1.646) 

(2.491) 

(3.177) 

(1.932) 

Figure  1  compares  the  predation  patterns  that  occurred  under 
natural  LD  and  reversed  LD  cycles.  Tukey  HSD  multiple 
comparisons  showed  that  under  natural  LD  the  mean  number  of  prey 
killed  at  night  was  significantly  higher  than  either  morning  (p<.0\)  or 
afternoon  (p<.0\).  Although  mean  number  of  prey  killed  was  higher 
during  evening  than  either  morning  or  afternoon,  the  differences  were 


JACK  PALMER  AND  LINDA  PALMER  33 


40  -1 


5   30 


0) 


0) 

^  20 

o 

u 


10 


Natural  LD 
Reversed  LD 


'  I  1  I  I 

Afternoon     Evening     Night     Morning 


Figure  1.  Circadian  predatory  behavior  patterns  under  lighting  conditions 
natural  and  reversed  LD. 


not  significant.  Kill  rates  for  evening  and  night  sessions  did  not  differ 
significantly  from  each  other.  Morning  and  afternoon  kill  rates  also 
did  not  differ  significantly  from  each  other. 

Under  reversed  LD  conditions,  the  pattern  of  predation  reversed 
from  that  exhibited  under  natural  LD.  Tukey  HSD  multiple 
comparisons  revealed  that  morning  predation  was  significantly  higher 
than  either  night  (p<.00\)  or  evening  predation  (p<.00l).  Afternoon 
predation  was  also  significantly  higher  than  either  night  (p<.00\)  or 
evening  predation  (p<.00\).  The  reversed  LD  condition  produced  no 
significant  differences  between  morning  and  afternoon  predation. 
However,  night  predation  was  significantly  higher  than  evening 
predation  (p<.05). 

Under  DD,  night  predation  was  significantly  higher  than  afternoon 
predation  (p<.01).  Under  LL,  no  significant  differences  were  found 
among  the  time  periods. 

The  data  for  the  initial  week  under  each  of  the  two  constant 
conditions,  LL  and  DD,  were  examined  to  see  to  what  degree  the 
natural  LD  pattern  persisted.  Figure  2  illustrates  that  the  natural 
predation  pattern  did  persist  during  the  first  week  of  LL  and  DD  with 
predation  rates  peaking  during  night  periods.  Tukey  HSD  multiple 
comparisons  determined  the  following  significant  differences.  Under 
DD,  night  predation  was  significantly  higher  than  after  noon  (p<.Q5) 


34 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


Afternoon     Evening     Night     Morning 


Figure  2.     Circadian  rhythm  of  predation  during  the  first  week  of  LL,  DD,  and 
natural  LD  conditions. 


predation.     Under  LL,  night  predation  was  significantly  higher  than 
morning  (jx.Ol),  afternoon  (p<.0\),  and  evening  (p<.05)  predation. 

Table  1  displays  the  mean  number  of  prey  killed  by  each  subject. 
Predation  rates  of  the  10  subjects  were  analyzed  with  a  one-way 
ANOVA  to  determine  if  predation  differed  among  subjects.  No 
significant  differences  were  found. 


DISCUSSION 


Alligators  as  young  as  2  months  of  age  are  capable  of  capturing 
very  active  prey.  The  evidence  accrued  in  this  study  suggests  that 
predatory  behavior  in  the  young  alligator  varies  as  a  function  of  light- 
mediated  circadian  rhythm.  Light  plays  a  critical  role,  as  predation 
during  dark  periods  was  significantly  higher  than  during  light  periods, 
whether  lighting  conditions  were  natural  or  reversed.  Predation  rates 
during  natural  and  reversed  LD  climaxed  during  the  second 
contiguous  time  period  of  darkness  (i.e.,  night  period  of  natural  LD 
and  afternoon  period  of  reversed  LD).  This  suggests  that,  in  the  wild, 
predatory  behavior  of  the  young  alligator  reaches  a  peak  during  the 
hours  before  dawn,  declines  sharply  after  daybreak,  remains  low 
throughout  the  day,  and  then  rises  steadily  after  nightfall.     Such  an 


JACK  PALMER  AND  LINDA  PALMER  35 

activity  cycle  is  similar  to  the  circadian  rhythms  of  alligator  motor 
activity  found  in  other  research  (Kavaliers,  1980;  Kavaliers  &  Ralph, 
1980,  1981;  Lang,  1976). 

Aschoff  (1989)  describes  circadian  rhythms  as  "self-sustaining 
oscillations",  and  the  test  for  such  persistence  is  a  recording  of  the 
activity  under  continuous  non-varying  illumination.  Under  DD,  a 
predation  pattern  similar  to  natural  LD  persisted  (i.e.,  night  predation 
was  significantly  higher  than  afternoon),  although  predation  was 
relatively  suppressed  during  all  time  periods  (see  Table  2). 
Examination  of  predation  rates  of  the  first  week  under  conditions  of 
LL  and  DD  showed  that  a  general  pattern  of  circadian  rhythm,  similar 
to  the  natural  LD  pattern,  persisted.  During  the  second  week  of  LL, 
the  pattern  became  arhythmic.  This  is  in  accord  with  Kavaliers  and 
Ralph's  (1980)  study  where  the  free-running  circadian  rhythm  of 
alligator  motor  activity  became  arhythmic  after  10-15  days  of  LL. 

The  suppression  of  predation  during  DD  may  have  been  due  to 
melatonin,  a  hormone  that  increases  during  periods  of  darkness 
(Galluscio,  1990)  and  appears  to  regulate  cycles  of  sleep  and  activity 
in  many  animals  (Wurtman  &  Lieberman,  1987).  Melatonin  is 
present  in  the  blood  of  alligators  (Gern,  Owens,  Ralph,  &  Roth, 
1978),  but  its  exact  role  is  not  known.  In  birds,  the  increase  in 
melatonin  appears  to  cause  a  decrease  in  activity  and  in  body 
temperature  (Binkley,  1979).  Although  alligators  are  poikilotherms, 
an  increase  in  melatonin  due  to  continuous  darkness  may  suppress 
predatory  behavior. 

Figure  3  provides  further  evidence  of  an  endogenous  circadian 
rhythm  of  predatory  behavior.  If  the  temporal  occurrence  of  the 
young  alligator's  predatory  behavior  was  solely  under  light  stimulus 
control  with  no  endogenous  circadian  rhythm,  then  an  abrupt  change 
in  predatory  behavior  should  occur  immediately  following  light 
reversal.  However,  Figure  3  demonstrates  a  gradual  transition  of 
predation  pattern  in  response  to  reversed  LD,  more  indicative  of  a 
circadian  rhythm  being  reprogrammed  by  the  zeitgeber  than  behavior 
cued  directly  by  light.  Alligator  predation  follows  a  circadian  rhythm 
and  appears  to  be  dependent  upon  light-dark  variation  for  entrainment. 

As  an  ancient  species  that  has  evolved  in  latitudes  subject  to 
seasonal  shifts  in  photoperiod,  the  alligator  is  an  excellent  subject  for 
the  study  of  endogenous  circadian  clocks  that  are  fine  tuned  by 
environmental  cues  (eg.,  light-dark,  temperature  variations).  Whether 
there  is  one  circadian  clock  or  a  number  of  subordinate  circadian 
clocks    each    kept    in    synchronization    with    the    others    by    natural 


36 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


0) 

en 


40  n 


30  - 


20- 


10 


Afternoon     Evening     Night     Morning 


Figure  3.    Transition  of  circadian  predatory  pattern  from  natural  LD  (solid  triangles) 
to  the  first  week  (open  circles)  and  fourth  week  (open  triangles)  of  reversed  LD. 


zeitgebers  can  only  be  determined  by  extensive  research.  Amphibious 
behavior,  solar  compass  time  compensation,  photoreceptor  sensitivity, 
motor  activity,  and  predatory  behavior  all  may  be  controlled  by  one 
"master"  circadian  clock,  or  each  behavior  may  have  its  own 
independent  circadian  clock.  Conversely,  all  of  the  above  behaviors 
may  reflect  variations  symptomatic  of  the  animals'  activity  cycles.  To 
resolve  these  issues,  it  would  be  necessary  to  measure  continuously 
and  concurrently  all  of  these  behaviors  under  controlled  laboratory 
conditions.  By  manipulating  light  and  temperature  cues,  it  may  be 
possible  to  produce  desynchronization  of  the  behaviors  from  each 
other  thereby  demonstrating  subordinate  circadian  clocks  for  some  or 
all  of  these  behaviors. 


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 


Grateful  acknowledgement  is  due  to  Ted  Joanen  and  the 
Rockefeller  Wildlife  Refuge,  Grand  Chenier,  LA  for  supplying  the 
alligators  for  this  research.  Special  acknowledgement  is  due  to  Dr 
Ethel  Tobach,  Dr  Ernest  G  Maples  and  two  anonymous  reviewers  for 
their  valuable  comments. 


JACK  PALMER  AND  LINDA  PALMER  37 

REFERENCES 

Aschoff,  J.  (1989).    Temporal  orientation:    Circadian  clocks  in  animals  and  humans. 

Animal  Behavior,  37,  881-896. 
Binkley,  S.  (1979).    A  timekeeping  enzyme  in  the  pineal  gland.    Scientific  American, 

240,  66-71. 
Coulson,  R.A.,  &  Hernandez,  T.  (1983).    Alligator  metabolism:    Studies  on  chemical 

reactions  in  vivo.   New  York:    Maxwell  House. 
Galluscio,  E.H.  (1990).    Biological  psychology.    New  York:    Macmillan  Publishing 

Co. 
Gem,  W.A.,  Owens,  D.W.,  Ralph,  C.L.,  &  Roth,  J.J.  (1978).    Plasma  melatonin  from 

extra-pineal  sites.   American  Zoologist,  18,  557. 
Griffiths,    R.A.    (1986).      Natural   environmental    cues    and    circadian    rhythms    of 

behaviour:    A  perspective.    Chronobiology-Intemational,  3,  247-253. 
Kavaliers,  M.  (1980).    Circadian  rhythm  of  extraretinal  photosensitivity  in  hatchling 

alligators,  Alligator  mississippiensis.    Photochemistry  and  Photobiology,  32,  67- 

70. 
Kavaliers,  M.,  &  Ralph,  C.L.  (1980).    Circadian  organization  of  an  animal  lacking  a 

pineal    organ;    the    young    alligator.    Alligator    mississippiensis.       Journal    of 

Comparative  Physiology  139,  287-292. 
Kavaliers,  M.,  &  Ralph,  C.L.  (1981).    Encephalic  photoreceptor  involvement  in  the 

entrainment  and  control  of  circadian  activity  of  young   American  alligators. 

Physiology  and  Behavior,  26,  413-418. 
Lang,  J.W.  (1976).     Amphibious  behavior  of  Alligator  mississippiensis:     Roles  of  a 

circadian  rhythm  and  light.    Science,  191,  575-577. 
Mcllhenny,   E.A.   (1976).      The  alligator's  life  history.      Society   for  the   Study   of 

Amphibians  and  Reptiles.   (Original  work  published  1935). 
Murphy,  P.  (1981).     Celestial  compass  orientation  in  juvenile  American  alligators 

{Alligator  mississippiensis).    Copeia,  3,  638-645. 
Pooley,  A.C.  (1989).    Food  and  feeding  habits.    In  C.A.  Ross  (Ed.),  Crocodiles  and 

alligators  (pp.  76-91).    New  York:    Facts  on  File,  Inc. 
Ross,  C.A.,  &  Magnusson,  W.E.  (1989).    Living  crocodilians.     In  C.A.  Ross  (Ed.), 

Crocodiles  and  alligators  (pp.  58-73).    New  York:    Facts  on  File,  Inc. 
Scott,  T.P.,  &   Weldon,  P.J.  (1990).   Chemoreception  in  the  feeding  behavior  of  adult 

American  alligators.  Alligator  mississippiensis.   Animal  Behavior,  39,  398-405. 
Weldon,  P.J.,  Swenson,  D.J..  Olson,  J.K.,  &  Brinkmeier,  W.G.  (1990).   The  American 

alligator    detects    food    chemicals    in    aquatic    and    terrestrial    environments. 

Ethology,  85,  191-198. 
Wurtman,  R.J.,  &  Lieberman,  H.  (1987).     Melatonin  secretion  as  a  mediator  of 

circadian  variations  in  sleep  and  sleepiness.   Integrative  Psychiatry,  5,  13-14. 


International  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology,  Vol.  7,  No.  1,  1994 


PORTIA  LABIATA,  A  CANNIBALISTIC  JUMPING 

SPIDER,  DISCRIMINATES  BETWEEN 

OWN  AND  FOREIGN  EGGSACS 

Robert  J.  Clark 

Robert  R.  Jackson 

University  of  Canterbury 


ABSTRACT:  Eggsac  recognition  was  investigated  in  Portia  labiata,  a  jumping  spider 
(Salticidae)  that  routinely  feeds  on  the  eggs  of  conspecifics,  but  does  not  normally  feed 
on  its  own  eggs.  In  laboratory  experiments,  we  demonstrate  that  P.  labiata  females  can 
discriminate  between  their  own  and  foreign  eggsacs.  The  cues  by  which  these 
discriminations  are  made  are  discussed. 


INTRODUCTION 

Various  types  of  parental  care  are  known  in  spiders,  including 
guarding  eggs  against  predators  (Eberhard,  1974;  Kessler  &  Fokkinga, 
1973;  Pollard,  1984;  Fink,  1986,  1987;  Willey  &  Adler,  1989),  providing 
food  for  young  (N0rgaard,  1956;  Bristowe,  1958;  Kullmann,  1972; 
Shear,  1970)  and  opening  the  eggsac  to  allow  emergence  of  spiderlings 
(Whitcomb  &  Eason,  1967).  Generally,  if  a  female  that  does  not  have 
eggs  is  offered  eggs  of  a  conspecific,  she  will  reject,  and  sometimes  eat, 
them  (Bonnet,  1940;  Palmgren,  1944;  but  see  N0rgaard,  1956;  Pollard, 
1984).    Yet  females  do  not  normally  eat  their  own  eggs. 

In  vertebrates,  the  stimuli  by  which  females  recognize  their  own 
offspring  have  been  well  studied  (Fletcher  &  Michener,  1987),  but  the 
stimuli  by  which  female  spiders  discriminate  between  their  own  eggs  and 
those  of  conspecifics  have  received  little  attention.  Previous  studies 
suggest  that  eggsac  discrimination  by  spiders  is  mediated  by  one  or  more 
of  four  cues  (Kraft,   1982):     (1)  tactile  cues  based  on  the  physical 


Address  correspondence  to  Robert  J.  Clark,  Department  of  Zoology,  University  of 
Canterbury,  Christchurch,  New  Zealand. 

38  ©  1994  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology 


ROBERT  CLARK  AND  ROBERT  JACKSON  39 

characteristics  of  the  eggsac;  (2)  chemical  cues  associated  with  the 
eggsac's  silk;  (3)  chemical  cues  associated  with  the  spider's  web;  (4)  cues 
based  on  the  geographic  location  of  the  eggsac,  the  web  or  both. 

Portia  is  a  web-building  jumping  spider  that  specializes  in  preying 
on  other  spiders,  including  conspecifics  (Jackson,  1992).  Portia  females 
enter  the  webs  of  conspecifics,  where  they  attack  or  sometimes  kill  the 
resident  female,  then  eat  any  eggsacs  that  are  left  behind  (Jackson  & 
Blest,  1982;  Jackson  &  Hallas,  1986).  Upon  encountering  eggsacs, 
Portia  females  open  them  by  chewing  and  tugging  with  their  chelicerae, 
then  using  their  front  pair  of  legs  to  rake  the  eggs  forward  into  their 
mouths  (Jackson  &  Blest,  1982). 

Portia  females  have  never  been  observed  eating  their  own  eggs.  Yet 
females  leave,  then  return  to  their  webs  during  the  incubation  period 
(Jackson  &  Blest,  1982).  Also,  incubating  Portia  females  in  nature  have 
been  observed  to  eat  eggs  of  conspecifics  (Jackson  &  Blest,  1982; 
Jackson,  unpubl.  data).  This  suggests  that  P.  labiata  females  have 
evolved  an  ability  to  recognize  their  own  eggs  or  web.  We  investigated 
this  hypothesis  using  a  representative  species,  P.  labiata,  from  Sri  Lanka. 


MATERIALS  AND  METHODS 

Standard  maintenance  procedures  were  used,  as  described  elsewhere 
(Jackson  &  Hallas,  1986).  Tests  were  carried  out  in  cages  with 
removable  glass  sides  (Fig  lA).  An  internal  metal  frame  was  positioned 
inside  each  cage  (Fig  IB).  Spiders  attached  their  webs  to  the  metal 
frame  instead  of  to  the  glass,  enabling  the  cages  to  be  opened  without 
damaging  webs. 

All  females  used  in  tests  were  randomly  selected  from  the  laboratory 
stock.  Though  a  given  female  was  used  in  more  than  one  (maximum  of 
two  tests)  test,  no  eggsac-female  pair  was  used  more  than  once.  Also,  no 
eggsac  was  used  more  than  once,  except  for  instances  in  which  it  had 
previously  been  paired  with  its  parent.  All  eggsacs  used  in  tests  were  of 
approximately  (maximum  difference  of  3  days)  matching  age. 

Before  each  test,  the  parent  spider  (test  spiders  and  spiders  that 
provided  foreign  eggsacs  and  webs)  was  deprived  of  contact  with  its 
eggsac  and  web  for  a  2-h  period.  After  the  2-h  period,  the  test  female 
was  placed  in  a  cage  containing  one  of  the  following  treatments:  (1)  the 
test  female's  own  eggsac  in  the  test  female's  own  web;  (2)  the  eggsac 
and  web  of  another  conspecific  female  ('foreign  eggsac  in  foreign  web'); 
(3)  the  test  female's  eggsac  in  another  conspecific  female's  web  ('own 
eggsac  in  foreign  web');  (4)  the  eggsac  of  another  conspecific  in  the  test 


40 


INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 


B 


Figure  1.  Cage  (30  cm  x  30  cm  x  30  cm)  used  in  testing  Portia  labiata  for  eggsac 
recognition.  A:  Wooden  outer  frame  with  sliding  glass  sides,  one  of  which  is  shown 
partially  raised.  Prey  were  introduced  through  the  four  holes  (plugged  with  corks)  on  top 
of  the  cage.  B:  Inner  metal  frame  that  is  slightly  smaller  than  the  inside  dimensions  of 
outer  cage.  Inner  frame  is  shown  separately  for  clarity,  but  is  normally  positioned  inside 
the  outer  wooden  frame. 


female's  web  ('foreign  eggsac  in  own  web').  The  test  consisted  of  leaving 
the  test  female  in  the  cage  for  one  week,  during  which  time  the  eggsac 
was  checked  daily  for  evidence  of  having  been  fed  upon. 

Treatments  3  and  4  were  obtained  each  time  by  taking  a  pair  of 
incubating  females'  webs  and  trading  the  eggsacs:  eggsac  of  female  1 
placed  in  web  of  female  2  as  close  as  possible  to  previous  location  of 
eggsac  of  female  2,  and  likewise  for  eggsac  of  female  2  placed  in  web 
of  female  1 . 


RESULTS 


Eggsacs  encountered  by  test  females  in  their  own  webs  were  treated 
differently  depending  on  whether  they  belonged  to  the  test  female  or 
another  conspecific  female  (x^=47.62,  N=  59):  test  females  in  their  own 
webs  resumed  guarding  their  own  eggsac  in  19  out  of  20  tests,  and  ate 
the  foreign  eggsac  in  only  6  out  of  12  tests. 

Test  females  in  foreign  webs  resumed  guarding  their  own  eggsacs 


ROBERT  CLARK  AND  ROBERT  JACKSON  41 

in  13  out  of  14  tests  and  ate  the  eggsacs  in  12  out  of  13  tests.  Eggsacs 
which  the  test  female  did  not  resume  guarding  were  invariably  eaten. 


DISCUSSION 

Evidently,  P.  labiata  females  can  discriminate  between  their  own 
eggsac  and  a  foreign  eggsac.  In  the  present  study,  females  usually 
guarded  their  own  eggs  and  ate  foreign  eggs.  How  widespread  eggsac 
recognition  abilities  may  be  in  salticids  is  unknown  because  species 
other  than  P.  labiata  have  not  yet  been  studied.  However,  an  ability  to 
recognize  their  own  eggs  may  be  especially  advantageous  in  Portia 
because  this  is  a  genus  of  salticids  known  to  feed  frequently  on  eggs  of 
other  spiders,  including  conspecifics. 

This  study  raises  questions  about  the  cues  by  which  P.  labiata 
distinguishes  between  its  own  and  foreign  eggsacs.  In  some  vertebrates, 
cues  from  the  geographic  locations  of  the  eggs  are  known  to  be 
important  (Colgan,  1983).  However,  for  P.  labiata,  cues  other  than 
location  must  be  of  primary  importance.  In  our  tests,  when  females'  own 
eggsacs  were  placed  in  foreign  webs,  they  were  not  placed  in  a  location 
comparable  to  their  original  positions  in  the  parent  webs  but,  instead,  as 
close  as  possible  to  the  previous  location  of  the  foreign  eggsac. 
Therefore,  if  the  location  of  the  eggsac  in  the  web  was  the  most 
important  cue  for  eggsac  recognition,  then  test  females  would  have  been 
expected  often  to  eat  their  own  eggsacs.  However,  test  females  usually 
guarded  their  own  eggsacs,  instead  of  eating  them,  regardless  of  whether 
they  were  in  foreign  webs  or  the  females'  own  webs.  Probably,  in  P. 
labiata,  eggsac  discrimination  is  based  primarily  on  chemical  cues. 

However,  it  is  interesting  that,  when  we  placed  foreign  eggsacs  in 
the  webs  of  test  females,  the  test  females  accepted  (i.e.,  did  not  eat)  the 
foreign  eggsac  half  the  time.  Yet,  when  test  females  encountered  foreign 
eggsacs  in  foreign  webs,  they  usually  ate  them.  This  suggests  that  cues 
from  the  female's  own  web  are  important  in  addition  to  cues  from  her 
eggsac.  It  is  probably  unlikely  in  nature  for  a  female  to  encounter 
foreign  eggs  in  her  own  web  and,  therefore,  it  might  be  advantageous  for 
females  to  be  reluctant  to  eat  eggsacs  encountered  in  their  own  webs, 
despite  dissenting  chemical  cues. 


42  INTERNATIONAL  JOURNAL  OF  COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 

We  thank  Andrew  McLachlan,  Mary  Whitehouse  and  Philip  Taylor 
for  useful  comments  on  the  manuscript  and  Tracey  Robinson  for  her 
invaluable  help  in  the  preparation  of  the  manuscript.  Financial  support 
was  provided  by  grants  from  the  National  Geographic  Society  (3226-85) 
and  the  U.S. -New  Zealand  Cooperative  Program  of  the  National  Science 
Foundation  (BNS  8657078).  Import  permits  were  provided  by  the  New 
Zealand  Ministry  of  Agriculture  and  Fisheries. 

REFERENCES 

Bonnet  P.  (1940).  L'instinct  maternal  des  araignees  a  I'epreuve  de  I'experimentation. 

Bulletin  Societe  d'Histoire   Naturelle  Toulouse,  81,  185-250. 
Bristowe,  W.S.  (1958).  The  World  of  Spiders.    London:  Collins. 

Colgan,  P.  (1983).    Comparative  social  recognition.    New  York:  John   Wiley  &  Sons. 
Eberhard,  W.G.  (1974).   Maternal  behaviour  in  a  South  American  Lyssomanes.  Bulletin 

of  the  British  Arachnological  Society,  i,  51. 
Farley,  C,  &  Shear,  W.A.  (1973).   Observations  on  the  courtship  behaviour  of  Lycosa 

carolinensis.  Bulletin  of  the  British  Arachnological  Society,  2,  153-158. 
Fink,  L.S.  (1986).    Costs  and  benefits  of  maternal  behaviour  in  the  green  lynx  spider 

{Oxyo^xdois.,  Puecitia  viridans).   Animal  Behaviour,  34,  1051-1060. 
Fink,  L.S.   (1987).   Green  lynx  spider  egg  sacs:  source  of  mortality  and  the  function  of 

female  guarding  (Araneae,  Oxyopidae).  Journal  of  Arachnology,  15,  231-239. 
Fletcher,  D.J.C.,  &  Michener,  CD.  (1987).  Kin  recognition  in  animals.  Chichester:  John 

Wiley  &  Sons. 
Jackson,  R.R.  (1992).  Eight-legged  tricksters:  spiders  that  specialize  at  catching  other 

spiders.    BioScience,  42,  590-598. 
Jackson,  R.R.,  &  Blest,  A.D.  (1982).  The  biology  of  Portia  fimbriata,  a  web  building 

jumping  spider  (Araneae,  Salticidae)  from  Queensland:  utilization  of  webs  and 

predatory  versatility.  Journal  of  Zoology,  196,  255-292. 
Jackson,  R.R.,  &  Hallas  S.E.A.  (1986).   Comparative  biology  of  Portia  africana,  Portia 

albimana,  Portia  fimbriata,  Portia  labiata,  Portia  schultzi,  araneophagic  jumping 

spiders    (Araneae,    Salticidae):    utilisation   of   webs,    predatory    versatility    and 

intraspecific  interactions.  New  Zealand  Journal  of  Zoology,  13,  423-489. 
Kessler,  A.,  &  Fokkinga.  A.  (1973).   Hymenopterous  parasites  in  eggsacs  of  spiders  of 

the  genus  Pardosa  (Araneida,  Lycosidae).    Tijdschrift  voor  Entomologie,  116,  43- 

61. 
Kraft,  B.  (1982).  The  significance  and  complexity  of  communication  in  spiders.  In  P.N. 

Witt  &  J.S.  Rovner  (Eds.),  Spider  communication:  mechanisms  and  ecological 

significance.    Princeton:  Princeton  University  Press. 
Kullman,  E.J.  (1972).    Evolution  of  .social  behavior  in  spiders  (Araneae:  Eresidae  and 

Theriididae).   American  Zoologist,  12,  419-426. 
N0rgaard  E.  (1956).  Environment  and  behavior  ot'Theridion  saxatile.  Oikos,  7,  159-162. 
Palmgren,  P.  (1944).     Uber  die  brutpfleginstincthandlung  der  wolfspinnen.  Societas 

Scientiarum  fennica-Commentationes  Biologicae,  9,  1-19. 


ROBERT  CLARK  AND  ROBERT  JACKSON  43 

Pollard  S.D.  (1984).  Egg  guarding  by  Clubiona  cambridgei  (Araneae,  Clubionidae) 

against  conspecific  predators.  Journal  of  Arachnology,  ]],  323-326. 
Shear  W.A.  (1970).  The  evolution  of  social  phenomena  in  spiders.  Bulletin  of  the  British 

Arachnological  Society,  1,  65-76. 
Whitcomb  W.H.,  Eason  R.  (1967).  Life  history  and  predatory  importance  of  the  striped 

lynx  spider  (Araneida,  Oxyopidae).  Proceedings  of  the  Arkansas  Academy  of 

Science,  21,  49-58. 
Willey,  M.B.,  &  Adler  P.H.  (1989).  Biology  of  Peucitia  viridans  (Aranae,  Oxyopidae) 

in  South  Carolina,  with  special  reference  to  predation  and  maternal  care.  Journal 

of  Arachnology,  17,  275-284. 


The  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology 

Announces  its  Seventh  International  Conference 
July  4-8,  1994  at  the  University  of  Sao  Paulo,  Brazil 


Registration  and  program  information  may  be  obtained  from: 


Cesar  Ades 

ISCP  Local  Organizing  Committee 
Dept.  Experimental  Psychology 
Av.  Prof.  Mello  Moraes  1721 
Sao  Paulo  05508-900   BRAZIL 

Fax:  81  38895 

E-mail:         cades@fox.cce.usp.br 


To  obtain  the  following  publications,  write  to  Ethel  Tobach,  American 
Museum  of  Natural  History,  Central  Park  West  at  79th  Street,  New 
York,  NY   10024-5192,  USA  (bitnet:  etthc@cunyvm;  Fax:  212  769 

5233). 

ISCP  PUBLICATIONS: 

Historical    Perspectives    and    Contemporary    Status    of   Comparative 

Psychology 
Advances  in  Comparative  Psychology 

Volume   1:     The  Comparative  Psychology  of  Natural  Resource 
Management:  Costa  Rica 

Volume  2:    Australian  People  and  Animals  in  Today's  Dreamtime 

AVAILABLE  FOR  MAILING  COST  ONLY: 

Experimental  Studies  of  Elementary  Reasoning:  Evolutionary, 
Physiological  and  Genetic  Aspects  of  Behavior  by  L.  V.  Krushinsky, 
late  Chair  of  Higher  Nervous  Activity  in  the  Biological  Faculty  of 
Moscow  State  University  (1990). 


INTERNATIONAL  SOCIETY  FOR 
COMPARATIVE  PSYCHOLOGY 

AN  AFFILIATE  OF  THE  INTERNATIONAL  UNION  OF  PSYCHOLOGICAL  SCIENCES 


1994  MEMBERSHIP  APPLICATION  FORM 

I I  New  Member        I I    Renewing  Member       I I 

(Must  complete  entire  form) 


Renewing  Member 

(Complete  sections  A&B) 


Change  of  Address 

(Complete  section  A) 


Al. 

APPLICANT  DETAILS 

LAST 

FIRST              OTHER  INITIAL              DEGREE(S) 

DEPARTMENT 

E-MAIL  ADDRESS 

INSTITUTION 

STREET 

CITY 

STATE                                         ZIP 

(I'OUNtRY 

TELEPHONE  (Include  International  Codes) 

(           )                          (           ) 

DAYTIME                                       FAX 

A2.  AREA  OF  INTEREST 

NEW  MEMBERS 
Please    ensure    that    your 
application  is  endorsed  by 
a  current  ISCP  non-student 
member; 

MaME  of  endorser  (Please  print) 

SIGNATURE  OF  ENDORSER 


STUDENT  MEMBERS 

For      continuing      student 
members  and  new  student 
applicants: 

SUPERVISORS  NAME  (Please 

prim) 

SUPERVISORS  SIGNATURE 

COURSE  OF  STUDY 

B.   MEMBERSHIP  CATEGORY 

All  categories  include  subscription  to  the  International  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology 
LJ         Professional       Individual  interested  in  the  teaching,  theoretical  development  and 
experimental  investigation  of  comparative  psychology 

n  New  Member  Entry  Level $30 

LJ   Renewing  Member   $50 

I    I         Retiree   Professional  member  who  retires  or  is  without  financial  support.    .  .  .  $30 
I — I         Family   Two  members  who  have  the  same  mailing  address  and  require  only 

one  subscription  to  the  International  Journal  of  Comparative  Psychology  ....  $50 
LJ         Student  Individual  enrolled  at  an  institution  of  higher  learning  in  a 

scientific  field  related  to  comparative  psychology $30 


PAYMENT  INFORMATION 

The  1994  membership  year  is  January  1  to  December  31,  1994.    We  only  accept 

payment  in  U.S.  dollars  drawn  on  U.S.  banks.      Please  make  cheques  payable  to 

the  International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology  and  send  to: 

Dr  S.A.  McFadden,  ISCP  Secretary/Treasurer 

Departnnent  of  Psychology  •  The  Univ.  of  Newcastle  •  NSW  2308  •  AUSTRALIA 

Fax  +61  (49)  216980  •  E-Mail:  McFadclen@psychology.newcastle.edu.au 


Instructions  to  Authors 

All  manuscripts  are  reviewed  by  two  readers  other  than  the  Editors.  Authors 
should  send  three  copies  of  their  manuscripts  to  either  their  National  Editor  or 
to  the  Editor  (Hughes).  One  diskette  copy  saved  in  ASCII  format  or  as  a 
standard  word  processor  file,  will  subsequently  need  to  be  submitted  to  the 
Editor  once  the  manuscript  is  accepted  for  publication.  Indicate  clearly  on  the 
diskette  the  author(s)  name,  file  format  and  program  version  number.  (Alternate 
arrangements  may  be  possible  for  those  authors  who  are  unable  to  provide  a 
diskette  copy). 

The  manuscript  should  have  a  cover  page  with  the  full  title,  the  author(s) 
name(s)  and  address(es)  and  should  indicate  the  person  with  whom  the  Journal 
may  correspond.  If  the  author(s)  has(ve)  an  electronic  mail  address  or  fax 
number,  these  should  be  given  in  a  covering  letter  accompanying  the 
manuscript.  The  abstract  should  be  on  a  separate  page.  All  articles  are 
published  in  English;  the  author(s)  should  also  submit  an  abstract  in  their 
language  if  it  does  not  require  non-Roman  type.  Author(s)  are  responsible  for 
the  accuracy  of  the  non-English  abstract.  Acknowledgements  should  appear  on 
a  separate  page  and  carry  the  title  of  the  article  and  the  author(s)  name(s).  If 
the  author(s)  prefer(s)  anonymity  in  the  review  process,  this  should  be  stated  in 
a  covering  letter  and  the  manuscript,  abstracts,  and  illustrative  material  should 
carry  only  the  title  of  the  article.  The  cover  sheet  and  acknowledgements  will 
not  be  sent  to  the  reviewers  in  this  instance. 

GENERAL  REQUIREMENTS:  Logical  organization  is  essential.  While 
headings  help  to  structure  the  content,  titles  and  headings  within  the  manuscript 
should  be  as  short  as  possible.  Tables  and  figures  should  not  be  redundant. 
Illustrative  material  should  be  self-explanatory  and  used  sparingly.  Tables  and 
figures  must  be  in  camera-ready  condition  and  include  separate  captions.  Only 
items  cited  in  manuscripts  should  be  listed  as  references.  Page  numbers  must 
be  provided  for  direct  quotations.  Footnotes  should  not  be  used  except  in 
special  circumstances  and  with  permission  of  the  Editor. 

MANUSCRIPTS  should  be  double-spaced,  the  original  on  white  bond  paper, 
with  margins  of  3  cm  on  all  four  edges  and  approximately  26  lines  to  the  page. 
Manuscripts  longer  than  20  typed  pages  (not  counting  figures,  tables,  and 
references)  will  only  be  considered  in  unusual  circumstances.  The  style  manual 
of  the  American  Psychological  Association  (3rd  Edition,  1983)  is  to  be 
followed.  If  the  manual  is  not  available,  authors  may  write  to  the  Editor 
(Hughes)  for  a  style  sheet.  Manuscripts  which  do  not  meet  the  style 
requirements  will  be  returned  for  retyping. 

REFERENCES  should  be  listed  on  a  separate  page  and  referred  to  in  the  text 
by  author(s)  and  year  of  publication  as  in  the  Publication  Manual  of  the 
American  Psychological  Association. 


International  Society  for  Comparative  Psychology