Skip to main content

Full text of "Interpersonal diagnosis of personality; a functional theory and methodology for personality evaluation"

See other formats


NTERPERSQ 


FUNCTIONAL    THEOR 
...D    METHODOLOGY    FOR 
PERSONflnTY    EVALUATION 


%- 


X 


Timothy  Leary 


Interpersonal  Diagnosis 

of 

Personality 


A  Functional  Theory  and  Methodology 
for  Personality  Evaluation 


TIMOTHY  LEARY 


DIRECTOR   OF   PSYCHOLOGY   RESEARCH 

KAISER    FOUNDATION    HOSPITAL 

OAKLAND,   CALIFORNIA 


Resource  Publications 

An  imprint  of  Wipf  and  Stock  Publishers 
199  West  8th  Avenue  •  Eugene  OR  97401 


Resource  Publications 

A  division  of  Wipf  and  Stock  Publishers 

199W8th  Ave,  Suites 

Eugene,  OR  97401 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Personality 

A  Functional  Theory  and  Methodology  for  Personality  Evaluation 

By  Leary,  Timothy 

Copyright©  195  7  by  Leary,  Timothy 

ISBN:  1-59244-776-7 

Publication  date  7/30/2004 

Previously  published  by  John  Wiley  &  Sons,  1957 


To 
Marianne  Leary 


Preface 


This  book  is  concerned  with  interpersonal  behavior,  primarily  as  ex- 
pressed and  observed  in  the  psychotherapeutic  setting.  Its  value  lies 
in  its  emphasis  on  the  complexity  and  variety  of  human  nature  and 
on  the  objectivity  and  clarity  of  the  empirical  procedures  it  sets  forth 
for  multilevel  diagnosis.  The  research  on  which  it  reports  was  made 
possible  by  grants  from  the  United  States  Public  Health  Service  and 
the  Kaiser  Foundation. 

The  interpersonal  factors  of  personality  are  those  conscious  or 
unconscious  processes  which  people  use  to  deal  with  others  and  to 
assess  others  and  themselves  in  relation  to  others.  The  aim  of  the  inter- 
personal machinery  of  personality  is  to  ward  off  anxiety  and  preserve 
self-esteem.  One  of  the  major  results  of  these  operations  is  to  create 
the  social  environment  in  which  each  person  lives. 

Everyone  tends  to  make  his  own  interpersonal  world.  Neurosis 
or  maladjustment  involves  the  limiting  of  one's  interpersonal  appara- 
tus and  the  compulsive  use  of  certain  inflexible,  inappropriate  inter- 
personal operations  which  bring  about  results  that  are  painful,  unsatis- 
factory, or  different  from  one's  conscious  goals.  Adjustment  is  char- 
acterized by  an  understanding  of  one's  personahty  structure,  by  the 
development  of  mechanisms  flexible  enough  to  deal  with  a  variety  of 
environmental  pressures,  and  by  the  management  of  one's  behavioral 
equipment  in  such  a  way  as  to  avoid  situations  where  the  mechanisms 
will  be  ineffective  or  damaged. 

Any  statement  about  human  nature,  however,  is  restricted  in 
meaning  unless  the  level  of  behavior  to  which  it  refers  is  made  clear. 
The  first  step  must  be  a  definition  of  levels  and  an  ordering  of  data  in 
terms  of  levels.  The  aim  of  the  research  work  described  in  this  book 
has  been  to  develop  a  multilevel  model  of  personality  and  to  present  a 
series  of  complex  techniques  for  measuring  interpersonal  expressions 
at  these  different  levels  of  personality.  A  conceptual  and  empirical 
method  for  converting  observations  of  interpersonal  behavior  is  set 
forth.  The  reader  will  encounter  new  theories  about  the  effect  of 
interpersonal  behavior,  the  meaning  of  fantasy  expressions,  the  social 
language  of  symptoms,  and  the  nature  and  functional  meaning  of  con- 
flict.  These  theories  and  systematic  procedures  constitute  the  Inter- 


vi  PREFACE 

personal  System  of  Peisonality,  developed  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation 
Psychology  Research  Project. 

The  approach  employed  might  be  called  a  dynarmc  behaviorism. 
There  are  two  dynamic  attributes.  The  first  refers  to  the  impact  one 
person  has  or  makes  in  interaction  with  others;  the  second  refers  to 
the  interaction  of  psychological  pressures  among  the  different  levels 
of  personality.  The  behavioristic  attributes  of  the  system  derive  from 
the  procedure  of  viewing  every  response  of  the  subject  (overt,  verbal, 
symbolic)  as  a  unit  of  behavior  which  is  classified  by  objective 
methods  and  automatically  sorted  into  the  appropriate  level  of  per- 
sonality. The  patterns  and  clusters  of  thousands  of  these  responses, 
sorted  into  different  levels,  are  then  converted  by  mathematical  tech- 
niques into  indices  and  into  a  multilevel  diagnostic  code  summary. 
These  are  then  related  to  clinical  events  or  prognoses.  In  the  develop- 
ment of  the  interpersonal  system  more  than  5,000  cases  (psychiatric, 
medical,  and  normal  controls)  have  been  studied  and  diagnosed. 

In  addition  to  describing  and  validating  the  process  of  interper- 
sonal diagnosis  in  the  psychiatric  clinic,  this  volume  demonstrates  how 
these  theories  and  methods  may  be  applied  in  four  other  practical  set- 
tings— in  the  psychiatric  hospital,  in  psychosomatic  medicine,  in 
industrial  management,  and  in  group  therapy. 

This  book  should  be  interpreted  in  the  light  of  its  environmental 
and  professional  contexts.  It  is  the  product  of  clinical  psychologists 
working  in  a  psychiatric  setting,  and  practical  answers  have  been 
required  of  the  interpersonal  system  at  each  stage  of  its  development. 
This  gives  the  book  its  functional  cast.  As  to  its  implications  for  the 
profession  of  psychology,  in  my  own  mind  at  least,  a  new  concept  of 
the  "clinical  psychologist-as-diagnostician"  has  emerged.  In  the 
Introduction,  I  have  detailed  the  genesis  of  the  research  which  has 
resulted  in  the  book,  and  have  set  forth  the  contributions  of  the  many 
people  who  have  helped  to  bring  it  to  fruition. 

Timothy  Leary 

Berkeley,  California 
October,  1956 


Contents 


Introduction 


Part  I 

Some  Basic  Assumptions  About  Personality 
Theory 

CHAPTER  PAGE 

1.  Interpersonal  Dimension  OF  Personality 3 

2.  Adjustment-iMaladjustment  Factors  in  Personality  Theory       17 

3.  Systematizing  the  Complexity  of  Personality       .        .        .33 

4.  Empirical  Principles  in  Personality  Research        ...       45 

5.  Functional  Theory  of  Personality 50 

6.  General  Survey  of  the  Interpersonal  and 

Variability  Systems 59 


Part  II 

The  Interpersonal  Dimension  of  Personality: 
Variables,  Levels,  and  Diagnostic  Categories 

Introduction 90 

7.  The  Level  of  Public  Communication: 

The  Interpersonal  Reflex 91 

8.  The  Level  of  Conscious  Communication: 

The  Interpersonal  Trait 132 

9.  The  Level  of  Private  Perception: 

The  Interpersonal  Symbol 154 

10.  The  Level  of  THE  Unexpressed:  Significant  Omissions   .        .  192 

11.  The  Level  of  Values:  The  Ego  Ideal 200 

12.  A  System  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis 207 

vii 


CONTENTS 
Part  III 


The  Variability  Dimension  of  Personality: 
Theory  and  Variables 


Introduction 240 

13.  The  Indices  OF  Variability .241 

Part  IV 
Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Personality 

14.  Theory  of  Multilevel  Diagnosis 265 

15.  Adjustment  Through  Rebellion:  The  Distrustful 

Personality 269 

16.  Adjustment  Through  Self-Effacement: 

The  Masochistic  Personality 282 

17.  Adjustment  Through  Docility:  The  Dependent 

Personality 292 

18.  Adjustment  Through  Cooperation: 

The  Overconventional  Personality 303 

19.  Adjustment  Through  Responsibility: 

The  Hypernormal  Personality 315 

20.  Adjustment  Through  Power:  The  Autocratic 

Personality 323 

21.  Adjustment  Through  Competition:  The  Narcissistic 

Personality 332 

22.  Adjustment  Through  Aggression:  The  Sadistic  Personality      341 

Part  V 
Some  Applications  of  the  Interpersonal  System 

Introduction 352 

23.  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Hospitalized  Psychotics      .        .354 

24.  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  in  Medical  Practice: 

Psychosomatic  Personality  Types 373 

25.  Analysis  of  Group  Dynamics  in  an  Industrial 

Management  Group 403 

26.  Predicting  and  Measuring  Interpersonal  Dynamics  in 

Group  Psychotherapy 426 


CONTENTS  ix 

PAGE 

Appendices 

1.  Illustrations  of  the  Measurement  of  Interpersonal 

Behavior  at  Level  I 439 

2.  The  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List       .        .        .        .455 

3.  The  Administration,  Scoring,  and  Validation  of  the 

Level  III-TAT 464 

4.  The  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report 480 

5.  Norms,  Conversion  Tables,  and  Weighted  Scores  Used  in 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis 493 

Index  of  Names 501 

Index  of  Subjects 503 


Figures 


1.  Continuum  of  the  Sixteen  Interpersonal  V^ariables        ....  65 

2.  Interpersonal  Behavior  in  Psychotherapy 68 

3.  Change  in  Behavior  in  Therapy 70 

4.  Seven  Generic  Areas  of  Personality 85 

5.  Interpersonal  Interactions  in  Group  Therapy 95 

6.  Categorization  of  Check-List  Items 135 

7.  Illustrative  Self-Diagnosis 138 

8.  Conscious  Description  of  Father 139 

9.  Conscious  Description  of  Mother 140 

10.  Conscious  Description  of  Self  and  Family  Members    ....  141 

11.  Patient's  Description  of  Therapist 142 

12.  Therapist's  Description  of  Patient 144 

13.  Pattern  of  Familial  Relations 146-47 

14.  Variation  in  Depth  of  Measures 151 

15.  Diagnosis  of  Walter  Mitty 174 

16.  Profile  of  TAT  Scores 176 

17.  Conscious  and  "Preconscious"  Profiles 178 

18.  Conflict  Between  Power  and  Weakness 179 

19.  Facade  of  Power  and  Responsibility 180 

20.  Facade  of  Weakness  and  Docility 180 

21.  Rigidly  Conventional  Profiles 183 

22.  Depth  Continuum  of  Personality  Levels 187 

23.  Rigid  Avoidance  of  Rebelliousness 194 

24.  Consistent  Omission  of  Rebellious  Themes 196 

25.  Docile  Subject  Idealizes  Strength 204 

16.  Summary  Scores  for  Overconventional  Patient 218 

27.  Diagnosis  of  Facade  Behavior 219 

28.  Illustration  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis 222-23 

29.  Diagnosis  of  Level  III  Behavior 224 

30.  Illustration  of  Multilevel  Diagnosis 226-27 

31.  Generic  Variability  Indices 250 

32.  Calculation  of  Discrepancy  Values 258 

33.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  I 380 

xi 


xii  FIGURES 

FIGURE  PAGE 

34.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  II 382 

35.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  III 384 

36.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  Normal  Controls 387 

37.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  Ulcer  Patients 388 

38.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  Hypertensive  Patients 390 

39.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  Obese  Women 392 

40.  Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 394 

41.  Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 395 

42.  Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 397 

43.  Psychiatric  Clinic  Sample 398 

44.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  "Neurotics" 399 

45.  Multilevel  Mean  Scores  of  "Psychotics" 401 

46.  Self-Descriptions  of  Four  Executives 406 

47.  Self-Deception  Indices  of  Four  Executives 407 

48.  Group  Dynamics  Booklet 411-17 

49.  Perceptions  by  General  Manager 418 

50.  Consensual  Diagnosis  of  General  Manager 420 

51.  Perceptions  by  Production  Manager 421 

52.  Consensual  Diagnosis  of  Production  Manager 422 

53.  Perceptions  by  Personnel  Manager 423 

54.  Consensual  Diagnosis  of  Personnel  Manager 424 

55.  Predictions  of  Interpersonal  Roles 429 

56.  Measurements  of  Interpersonal  Roles 430 

57.  Diagram  of  Five  Measures  of  Personality 432-33 

58.  Two  Contrasting  MMPI  Profiles 442 

59.  The  Level  I  Diagnosis 444 

60.  Summaries  of  Interpersonal  Behavior 452 

61.  The  Diagnostic  Booklet 482-88 

62.  Multilevel  Profile  Before  and  After  Psychotherapy      .        .        .        .491 


Tables 


TABLE  PAGE 

1.  Operational  Definition  of  Five  Levels  of  Personality    .        .        .        .81 

2.  Percentage  of  Diagnostic  Types  (Level  I-M) 129 

3.  Percentage  of  Diagnostic  Types  (Level  II-C) 152 

4.  Illustrative  Classification  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  at  the  Symbolic 

or  Projective  Level 170 

5.  Percentage  of  Diagnostic  Types  (Level  III-T) 190 

6.  Three  Elements  of  Diagnosis  of  Personality:  Classification, 

Profiles,  and  Report 214 

7.  The  Adaptive  and  Maladaptive  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Types  .        .    220 

8.  Median  Interpersonal  Self-Description  Score  for  Six  MMPI 

Clinical  Groups 231 

9.  Operational  Redefinition  of  Psychiatric  Categories  in  Terms  of 

Interpersonal  Operations 233 

10.  Informal  Listing  of  the  Twelve  Generic  Variability  Indices  .        .        .252 

11.  Operational  Definition  of  Forty-eight  Indices  of  Variation  .        .       254-56 

12.  Key  to  Numbers  and  Letters  Employed  in  Coding  Variability  Indices     256 

13.  Horizontal  (Lov)  and  Vertical  (Dom)  Values  for  Each  Octant  .        .    260 

14.  All  Possible  Discrepancies  Around  the  Pair  \-l  and  Their  Magnitudes    260 

15.  Illustration  of  the  Grouping  of  All  Possible  Discrepancies  Involving 

the  Diagnostic  Codes  1  and  / 261 

16.  Percentage  of  Rebellious-Distrustful  Personalities  (Level  I-M)  .  .  280 

17.  Percentage  of  Rebellious-Distrustful  Personalities  (Level  II-C)     .  .281 

18.  Percentage  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic  Personalities  (Level  I-M)  .  290 

19.  Percentage  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic  Personalities  (Level  II-C)  .  291 

20.  Percentage  of  Docile-Dependent  Personalities  (Level  I-M)  .        .  .  299 

21.  Percentage  of  Docile-Dependent  Personalities  (Level  II-C)        .  .  300 

22.  Percentage  of  Cooperative-Overconventional  Personalities 

(Level  I-M) 312 

23.  Percentage  of  Cooperative-Overconventional  Personalities 

(Level  II-C) 313 

24.  Percentage  of  Responsible-Hypernormal  Personalities  (Level  I-M)     .    321 

25.  Percentage  of  Responsible-Hypernormal  Personalities  (Level  II-C)    .    322 

26.  Percentage  of  Managerial-Autocratic  Personalities  (Level  I-M)  .        .330 

xiii 


^j^  TABLES 

TABLE  ^^^^ 

27.  Percentage  of  Managerial- Autocratic  Personalities  (Level  II-C)  .     331 

28.  Percentage  of  Competitive-Narcissistic  Personalities  (Level  I-M)  .     338 

29.  Percentage  of  Competitive-Narcissistic  Personalities  (Level  II-C)  .     340 

30.  Percentage  of  Aggressive-Sadistic  Personalities  (Level  I-M)       .  .     349 

31.  Percentage  of  Aggressive-Sadistic  Personalities  (Level  II-C)       .  .350 

32.  Level  I  Diagnoses  Assigned  to  148  Patients  in  the  Three 

Psychotic  Samples 356 

33.  Level  II-C  Diagnosis  of  46  Patients  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples    .  357 

34.  Level  III-T  Diagnosis  of  38  Patients  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples  358 

35.  The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups 

at  Level  I-M 381 

36.  The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups 

at  the  Level  of  Conscious  Self-Description  (Level  II-C)         .         .     383 

37.  The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups 

at  the  Level  of  "Preconscious"  Expression  (Level  Ill-T  [Hero])     .     385 

38.  Illustrative  Calculation  of  MMPI  Indices  for  Measuring  Symptomatic 

Behavior   (Level  I-M) 443 

39.  Illustration  of  the  Calculations  for  Determining  the  Level  I  Profile  for 

a  "Neurotic"  Patient,  SN 453 

40.  Interpersonal  Check  List,  Form  4,  Words  Arranged  by  Octant  and 

Intensity 456-57 

41.  Test-Retest  Correlations,  Form  IIIa,  by  Octant  and  Sixteenth     .        .     461 

42.  Average  Intervariable  Correlation  as  a  Function  of  Their  Separation 

Around  the  Circle 462 

43.  ICL  Means  and  Standard  Deviations  for  Psychiatric  Outpatients  .         .     463 

44.  Guide  to  Assigning  Interpersonal  Ratings  to  Ten  TAT  Stories 

(Level  III-T) 466 

45.  Molar  Rating  Sheet 471 

46.  Means  and  Sigmas  of  Normative  Group  for  Level  III-T  Hero  and 

"Other" 472 

47.  Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Dominance- 

Submission  Between  Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis 
to  the  Kind  of  Change  in  Self-Diagnosis  of  Dominance-Submission 
on  Pre-Post  Tests  for  23  Psychotherapy  Patients      ....    474 

48.  Chi-Square  ...  for  40  Discussion  Group  Controls      ....     475 

49.  Chi-Square  ...  for  Combined  Samples  of  23  Psychotherapy  Patients 

and  40  Obesity  Patients 475 

50.  Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Love-Hostility 

Between  Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind 
of  Change  in  Self-Diagnosis  of  Love-Hostility  on  Pre-Post  Tests 
for  23  Psychotherapy  Patients 476 


TABLES 

XV 

TABLE  PAGE 

51.  Chi-Square  ...  for  40  Discussion  Group  Controls      ....    476 

52.  Chi-Square  ...  for  Combined  Samples  of  23  Psychotherapy  Patients 

and  40  Obesity  Patients 477 

53.  Chi-Square  Relating  the  Amount  of  Discrepancy  Between  Conscious 

Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  Amount  of  Temporal  Change 

in  Self-Diagnosis  for  81  Discussion  Group  Controls  ....     478 

54.  Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard 

Scores  at  Level  I-M 494 

55.  Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard 

Scores  at  Level  II-C 495 

56.  Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard 

Scores  at  Level  III-TAT  (Hero) 496 

57.  Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard 

Scores  at  Level  III-TAT  (Other) 497 

58.  Weighted  Scores  for  Measuring  Discrepancy  Between  Two  Diag- 

nostic Codes  Indicating  Kind  and  Amount  of  Difference  Between 
Levels  or  Tests,  and  for  Comparing  Codes  Where  One  Is  of 
Extreme  and  the  Other  of  Moderate  Intensity  ....       498-99 


Introduction 


In  the  past,  the  complexity  of  personality  data,  particularly  as  it  is 
observed  in  the  clinical  setting,  has  led  to  a  relative  neglect  of  em- 
pirical studies  and  to  an  emphasis  on  anecdotal,  speculative  accounts. 
Where  objective  investigations  have  been  undertaken,  they  have 
tended  to  be  analyses  which  employed  a  single  testing  instrument. 
This  is  a  result  of  the  sociological  development  of  the  testing  psy- 
chologist's role. 

The  original  and  basic  aim  of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychology 
Research  was  (and  still  is)  the  study  of  "process  in  psychotherapy." 
The  first  steps  in  this  direction  involved  the  construction  of  a  sys- 
tematic way  of  viewing  personality  structure  before  therapy.  This 
model  system  is  necessary  to  predict  what  will  happen  in  therapy  and 
to  measure  change  in  structure  during  and  after  therapy.  This  book 
presents  such  a  system  and  some  of  its  diagnostic  and  prognostic 
features. 

The  United  States  Public  Health  Service  supported  the  research 
project  by  a  series  of  six  annual  grants,  from  1950  to  1954,  under  the 
directorship  of  Hubert  S.  Coffey  and  Dr.  Saxton  T.  Pope,  Jr.,  and 
from  1954  to  1956  under  the  direction  of  Timothy  Leary.  In  addition 
to  serving  as  the  first  director,  Hubert  Coffey  has  been  chief  advisor 
since  the  first  days  of  the  project.  Dr.  Pope  provided  research 
facilities  and  clinical  wisdom,  and  was  of  signal  help  in  developing  the 
concept  of  variability  indices,  discussed  in  Chapter  13. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  contributed  substantially  to  the  research 
during  the  years  1950-1954,  and  from  November  1954  assumed  major 
support  of  the  core  project.  Dr.  Harvey  Powelson  became  the 
director  of  the  research  project  in  1951.  He  has  given  clinical  advice, 
theoretical  counsel,  and  administrative  support  throughout  the  dura- 
tion of  the  research. 

In  its  development,  the  interpersonal  system  of  personality  has 
been  influenced  by  many  collaborating  psychologists  and  psychiatrists. 
It  is  impossible,  in  a  cooperative,  creative  enterprise  of  this  scope  to 
accord  specific  credit  for  all  contributions,  and  the  following  acknowl- 
edgments indicate  only  the  major  indebtedness.  Those  whose  names 
are  listed  below  should  not,  however,  be  held  accountable  for  any 


^^••-  INTRODUCTION 

weaknesses  in  the  theoretical  design.  Full  responsibility  for  the  present 
version  of  the  system  is  assumed  by  the  author. 

The  basic  notion  of  the  interpersonal  classification  system  (the 
circle)  was  developed  in  1948-1949  by  Hubert  Coffey,  Mervin  Freed- 
man,  Timothy  Leary,  and  Abel  Ossorio.  The  same  group  was  respon- 
sible for  the  original  tripartite  definition  of  levels.  The  psychotherapy 
groups  which  provided  the  original  data  for  classification  of  inter- 
personal reflexes  were  organized  with  the  help  and  sponsorship  of 
J.  Raymond  Cope,  of  the  Unitarian  Church  of  Berkeley. 

Dr.  Mary  Sarvis,  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychiatric  Clinic,  lent  her 
diagnostic  and  therapeutic  knowledge  to  the  research  group  with  un- 
sparing generosity. 

Mervin  Freedman  was  a  major  participant  in  every  stage  of  theo- 
retical and  methodological  development  from  1948  to  1953.  His 
thoughtful,  analytic  approach  provided  balance  and  good  sense. 

Rolfe  LaForge  is  responsible  for  the  successful  aspects  of  the 
statistical  and  methodological  work.  From  1950  to  1954,  he  directed 
the  testing  program,  the  IBM  research,  the  check-list  studies,  and 
served  as  statistical  consultant. 

Martin  Levine,  Blanche  Sweet,  Herbert  Naboisek,  and  Ellen 
Philipsborn  Tessman  made  theoretical  contributions  and  aided  in  the 
processing  of  data. 

Jean  Walker  McFarlane  was  an  original  sponsor  and  advisor  of  the 
research  project  and  contributed  continuous  editorial  and  practical 
assistance. 

Arthur  Kobler  of  the  Pinel  Foundation  Hospital,  Seattle,  has  em- 
ployed the  diagnostic  system  in  his  studies  of  psychotic  patients.  The 
combination  of  his  empirical  help  and  theoretical  counsel  has 
strengthened  this  book  in  several  areas. 

Bernard  Apfelbaum  collaborated  in  the  early  stages  of  the  oscilla- 
tion-variability theory.  He  also  provided  ratings  of  interpersonal 
behavior,  as  did  Wanda  Bronson,  Albert  Shapiro,  and  Marvin 
Spanner. 

Frank  Barron  has  served  since  1950  as  official  and  unofficial  con- 
sultant to  the  research  project.  He  helped  design  the  original  test 
battery  and  provided  valuable  editorial  and  methodological  assistance. 
Psychotherapy  groups  studied  by  the  research  project  were  in 
charge  of  Dr.  Jean  Neighbor,  Mary  Darby  Rauch,  Shirley  Hecht, 
Mervin  Freedman,  Stephen  Rauch,  Abel  Ossorio,  Dr.  Harvey  Powel- 
son,  Robert  Suczek,  Hubert  Coffey,  Patrick  SuUivan,  and  Richard  V. 
Wolton.  Richard  Wolton  also  lent  his  assistance  in  the  collection  of 
data  and  in  manuscript  preparation. 


INTRODUCTION  xlx 

A  most  important  aspect  of  the  interpersonal  system  is  that  the 
test  administration,  scoring,  and  rating  of  tests — as  well  as  the  deter- 
mination of  the  multilevel  diagnoses  and  the  indices  of  conflict — are 
accomplished  by  highly  trained  technicians  who  are  not  professional 
psychologists.  The  technical  staff  responsible  for  the  multilevel 
diagnoses  of  the  5,000  cases  on  which  this  book  is  based,  includes  Anne 
Apfelbaum,  Elizabeth  Asher,  Mary  della-Cioppa,  Roberta  Held, 
Charlotte  Kaufmann,  Joan  Harvey  LaForge,  Helen  Lane,  and  Bar- 
bara Lennon  NichoUs.  Gloria  Best  Martin  was  Research  Administra- 
tor for  the  years  1950-1952. 

The  countless  administrative  decisions  necessary  to  maintain  the 
day-to-day  operations  of  the  research  project  have  been  handled  with 
competence  by  Miss  Helen  Lane.  She  has  had  final  executive  respon- 
sibility for  data  collection,  office  management,  and  manuscript 
preparation. 


I 


Some  Basic  Assumptions  About 
Personality  Theory 


Interpersonal  Dimension  of  Personality 


The  twentieth  century  may  well  find  historical  status  as  the  epoch  in 
which  man  began  to  study  himself  as  a  scientific  phenomenon.  This 
development,  inaugurated  mainly  by  Sigmund  Freud  around  the  year 
1900,  has  brought  about  an  impressive  growth  in  the  so-called  human- 
ist disciplines — psychiatry,  psychology,  anthropology,  sociology.  The 
hour  is  yet  too  early  to  begin  writing  the  chronicles  of  our  time,  but 
certain  trends,  now  clearly  evident,  allow  tentative  predictions. 

The  study  of  human  nature  appears,  at  this  mid-century  point,  to 
be  shifting  from  an  emphasis  on  the  individual  to  an  emphasis  on  the 
individual-in-relation-to-others.  During  the  last  fifty  years  the  sub- 
ject matter  of  psychiatry,  for  example,  has  moved  away  from  case 
history  and  symptomatic  labels  and  proceeded  in  the  direction  of 
social  interaction  analysis  and  psychocukural  phenomena.  The  physi- 
calistic  therapies,  such  as  electro-shock  and  neurosurgery,  seem  to 
have  worked  with  little  theoretical  justification  against  these  scientific 
currents  of  the  time.^ 

As  late  as  twenty  years  ago  the  psychiatric  literature  was  saturated 
with  concepts  that  were  oriented  towards  the  nonsocial  aspects  of  per- 
sonality— man  in  relation  to  his  instinctual  past  (Freud),  his  racial  past 
(Jung).  The  psychological  laboratories  at  the  same  time  buzzed  with 
experiments  on  achievement,  intelligence,  temperament,  and  learning 
processes  of  the  individual  animal  or  human  being. 

Today,  theoretical  events  have  taken  a  different  turn.  Man  is 
viewed  as  a  uniquely  social  being,  always  involved  in  crucial  inter- 
actions with  his  family  members,  his  contemporaries,  his  predecessors, 
and  his  society.  All  these  factors  are  seen  as  influencing  and  being  in- 
fluenced by  the  individual.  The  new  direction  is  marked  by  a  series  of 
new  conceptual  guide  posts  from  communication  theory,  cultural 
anthropology,  and  neop<;ychoanalysis.  We  possess  a  new  bibliography 

*  The  research  on  neuropsychological  relations  accomplished  at  Tulane  University 
under  the  direction  of  Robert  G.  Heath  is  a  notable  exception  to  this  generalization. 


4  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

of  guide  books  pointing  out  the  approaching  scientific  horizons  and 
relating  them  to  the  past. 

There  is  one  concept  which  finds  such  wide  and  repeated  expression 
in  the  current  literature  that  it  has  taken  on  the  debatable  character 
of  a  motto.  This  is  the  term  interpersonal  relations.  Introduced  by  the 
American  psychiatrist  Harry  Stack  Sullivan,  it  has  become  so  popular 
that,  at  times,  it  appears  destined  to  join  those  ill-fated  concepts 
rendered  meaningless  by  the  frequency  and  pious  generality  of  their 
usage. 

The  interpersonal  theory  is  clearly  a  product  of  the  converging 
theoretical  trends  of  the  time.  It  has  many  important  implications  for 
all  the  humanistic  disciplines. 

This  book  and  the  research  which  it  summarizes  take  as  a  starting 
point  the  interpersonal  dimension  of  personality.  We  shall  trace  in 
the  following  chapters  a  theory,  a  measurement  methodology,  and  a 
psychological  diagnostic  system  based  primarily  on  interpersonal  be- 
havior. It  seems  appropriate,  therefore,  to  take  as  the  first  question  for 
consideration  the  definition  of  the  basic  term,  interpersonal. 

What  Is  Interpersonal  Behavior? 

Behavior  which  is  related  overtly,  consciously,  ethically,  or  sym- 
bolically to  another  human  being  (real,  collective,  or  imagined)  is 
interpersonal.  This  is  a  short  but  complex  definition.  Most  of  the 
succeeding  pages  will  be  devoted  to  its  elaboration. 

Let  us  consider  some  examples  of  human  behavior  in  the  light  of 
this  definition.  The  report  from  a  reliable  observer  "George  insulted 
his  father"  is  clearly  interpersonal.  It  tells  how  George  related  to  his 
father,  what  he  did  to  his  father.  The  finding  "George  says  he  is  a 
friendly  person"  comes  from  a  different  observation  point,  the  sub- 
ject's self-description,  but  is  still  clearly  interpersonal.  It  tells  how 
George  perceives  his  motives  toward  other  people.  Also  interpersonal 
is  the  inference  made  on  the  basis  of  dream  or  fantasy  material  "George 
dreams  that  his  mother  is  protecting  him."  This  refers  to  a  fantasied 
relationship  between  the  subject  and  another  person.  These  descrip- 
tions of  different  aspects  of  the  subject's  behavior,  which  we  call 
protocol  statements,  are  the  basic  data  on  which  we  build  a  science  of 
personality.  They  describe,  at  three  different  levels  of  observation, 
the  subject's  interpersonal  relations. 

Another  dimension  of  personality  is  reflected  in  the  statements 
"George  acts  impulsively,"  "George  says  he  is  not  depressed,"  "George 
dreams  of  hatboxes."  These  descriptions  are  taken  from  the  same 
three  levels  of  observation — the  outsider's  report,  self-report,  and 
dreams — but  they  are  not  directly  interpersonal.    Impulsivity,  opti- 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY  5 

mism,  and  a  symbolic  concern  with  containers  have  figured  in  certain 
personality  theories  and  have  some  importance  in  the  understanding 
of  personality.  Such  descriptions  are  noninterpersonal  because  they 
do  not  refer  to  the  subject's  relationship  to  other  people.  They  may 
be,  and  probably  are,  indirectly  interpersonal.  If  we  investigate 
further  we  might  learn  that  George  acts  impulsively  to  impress  others 
with  his  strength,  that  he  says  he  is  not  depressed  to  prove  that  he 
does  not  need  psychotherapy,  and  that  he  has  a  vague  childhood 
memory  of  his  mother  bringing  him  lunch  in  a  hatbox.  The  non- 
interpersonal  thus  becomes  interpersonal;  the  personal  characteristics 
take  on  a  social  meaning  and  reflect  his  relationships  with  others. 

We  shall  subsequently  see  that  much  of  the  conceptualization  in 
psychology  and  the  nomenclature  of  psychiatry  has  been  noninterper- 
sonal. Terms  such  as  depressed,  impulsive,  and  inhibited,  for  example, 
refer  to  characteristics  that  possess  maximum  meaning  when  their  inter- 
personal purpose  is  added.  From  the  restricted  and  partisan  inter- 
personal point  of  view,  the  functional  value  of  such  a  popular  diag- 
nostic phrase  as  "the  patient  acts  depressed"  is  really  not  very  great 
until  we  add,  overtly  or  implicitly,  the  social  implication.  We  make 
such  a  phrase  more  meaningful  when  we  designate  the  interpersonal 
context  or  the  interpersonal  impact  of  the  action — "to  get  the  psy- 
chiatrist's sympathy"  or  "to  show  his  parents  how  badly  he  feels 
they  have  treated  him." 

Psychologists  or  psychiatrists  who  employ  interpersonal  concepts 
are  generally  characterized  by  an  obsessive  attention  to  the  social  im- 
plications of  the  subject's  performance.  They  tend  to  view  themselves 
as  engaged  in  a  complex  relationship  with  the  subject  (or  patient)  and 
are  particularly  concerned  with  the  social  pressure  which  the  subject 
is  generating — the  impression  he  is  attempting  to  make  upon  them. 

The  interpersonal  psychologist  generally  carries  away  from  an 
interview  or  a  testing  session  a  diagnosis  centering  not  on  the  patient's 
intelligence  or  his  symptoms,  but  rather  on  the  social  machinery  which 
the  patient  put  into  action  during  the  session.  In  most  clinical  situa- 
tions a  numerical  IQ  index  is  of  limited  functional  value.  The  clinician 
working  from  the  interpersonal  viewpoint  would  be  more  likely  to 
stress  not  the  patient's  IQ,  but  the  fact  that  "the  patient  acts  in  a  wise 
manner  and  attempts  to  create  the  impression  of  intelligence,"  or,  in 
another  case,  "the  patient  presents  a  fa9ade  of  docile  simplicity,  acts 
as  though  he  were  uninformed  and  eager-to-be-taught." 

SoTne  Noninterpersonal  Systems  of  Psychology 

The  interpersonal  system  presented  in  this  book  addresses  itself  to  a 
narrow,  limited  slice  of  human  behavior.  There  are  many  other  facets 


6  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

of  human  activity  which  have  attracted  the  interest  and  energy  of 
psychologists.  In  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  we  omit  or  ignore 
about  nine-tenths  of  these  activities  and  concentrate  rather  single- 
mindedly  on  one  dimension — the  interpersonal.  We  have  restricted 
our  theory  to  social  behavior  because  we  believe  this  to  be  the  area 
of  psychology  which  is  most  crucial  and  functionally  important  to 
human  happiness  and  human  survival.  Our  reasons  for  making  this 
assertion  will  be  detailed  in  a  later  section. 

In  restricting  our  studies  to  one  source  of  data  we  fail  to  take  into 
account  hundreds  of  important  variables  which  characterize  the 
individual.  Height,  weight,  age,  appearance,  and  motoric  patterns  are 
all  factors  which  have  some  value  in  predicting  behavior.  All  the 
physiological  aspects  of  the  individual  are  left  out  of  our  system. 

Sociological  factors  also  contribute  to  the  understanding  of  per- 
sonality and  carry  clear-cut  interpersonal  implications.  We  have  been 
unable,  so  far,  to  include  these  factors  in  our  investigations. 

Moreover,  we  have  found  it  necessary  to  omit  most  of  the  variables 
which  have  had  the  highest  priority  for  most  psychologists — intelli- 
gence, interest  patterns,  political  and  culmral  attitudes,  and  the 
variables  of  sensation  and  perception. 

Academic  and  experimental  psychology  has  traditionally  focused 
on  the  noninterpersonal  aspects  of  behavior.  Psychophysical  experi- 
ments, learning  theories,  and  intelligence  and  aptitude  studies  have 
monopolized  the  majority  of  the  chapters  in  psychological  texts. 
These  areas  are  left  completely  untreated  in  the  system  of  personality 
presented  in  this  book. 

We  are  concerned,  therefore,  with  a  limited  sector  of  the  wide 
circle  of  human  behavior.  We  concentrate  simply  on  the  way  in 
which  the  individual  deals  with  others — his  actions,  thoughts,  fantasies, 
and  values  as  they  relate  to  others.  In  addition  to  restricting  our  atten- 
tion to  interpersonal  activity,  there  is  a  further  qualification.  We  can- 
not hope  to  include  the  entire  range  of  the  individual's  social  behavior, 
but  will  apply  most  of  our  energies  to  the  task  of  understanding  and 
predicting  the  subject's  interpersonal  behavior  in  one  specific  environ- 
mental context — his  relationship  to  a  psychiatric  clinic. 

Some  Interpersonal  Theories  of  Personality 

We  have  seen  that  in  the  last  twenty  years  the  cultural  and  social 
factors  of  human  nature  have  become  the  object  of  widespread  scien- 
tific attention.  Sociologists  and  anthropologists  have  been  actively 
applying  psychiatric  concepts  to  their  data  with  mixed  results.  Entire 
primitive  societies  have  been  diagnosed  as  paranoid,  or  typed  in  terms 
of  the  ways  in  which  they  feed  their  young. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY  7 

At  the  same  time,  on  the  other  side  of  the  professional  fence,  several 
psychiatrists  have  assimilated  the  cultural  into  their  thinking.  Major 
revisions  of  orthodox  Freudian  concepts  have  developed.  Three  of 
the  most  successful  of  these  personaHty  theorists,  Horney,  Fromm, 
and  Sullivan,  have  rejected  the  instinct  theory  and  developed  socially 
oriented  structures  of  their  own.  A  fourth,  Erik  H.  Erikson,  has  con- 
structed an  impressive  system  integrating  social  phenomena  into  the 
Freudian  libido  theory. 

Karen  Horney  began  publishing  in  1937  a  series  of  important  books 
in  which  she  has  developed  a  characterological  approach  to  person- 
ality. She  has  described  her  dissatisfaction  with  the  instinct  theory  and 
her  own  conceptual  solutions  in  great  detail.  In  her  earliest  work  she 
contended  that  "neuroses  are  brought  about  by  cultural  factors" — 
which,  more  specifically,  meant  that  neuroses  are  generated  by  dis- 
turbances in  human  relationships. 

In  the  years  before  I  wrote  The  Neurotic  Personality  I  pursued  another  line 
of  research  that  followed  logically  from  the  earlier  hypothesis.  It  revolved 
around  the  question  as  to  what  the  driving  forces  are  in  neuroses.  Freud  had 
been  the  first  to  point  out  that  these  were  compulsive  drives.  He  regarded 
these  drives  as  instinctual  in  nature,  aimed  at  satisfaction  and  intolerance  of 
frustration.  Consequently  he  believed  that  they  were  not  confined  to  neuroses 
per  se  but  operated  in  all  human  beings.  If,  however,  neuroses  were  an  out- 
growth of  disturbed  human  relationships,  this  postulation  could  not  possibly  be 
valid.  The  concepts  I  arrived  at  on  this  score  were,  briefly,  these.  Compulsive 
drives  are  specifically  neurotic;  they  are  born  of  feelings  of  isolation,  helpless- 
ness, fear,  and  hostility,  and  represent  ways  of  coping  with  the  world  despite 
these  feelings;  they  aim  primarily  not  at  satisfaction  but  at  safety;  their  compul- 
sive character  is  due  to  the  anxiety  lurking  behind  them.  Two  of  these  drives- 
neurotic  cravings  for  affection  and  for  power— stood  out  at  first  in  clear  relief 
and  were  presented  in  detail  in  The  Neurotic  Personality.   (4,  p.  11) 

Later  books  presented  increasingly  sophisticated  attempts  to  de- 
lineate the  neurotic  character  structure.  Homey  has  listed  many  types, 
trends,  and  conflicting  attitudes  to  this  end.  All  of  these  constructs 
concern  the  individual's  reactions  to  others.  At  the  time  of  her  death, 
Horney's  systematizing  efforts  were  far  from  completed.  The  shifts 
in  her  flexible  development  have  created  the  appearance  of  a  brilliant 
disorganization.  An  over-all  survey  of  her  publications,  however, 
reveals  an  internal  consistency  and  a  steady  progress  towards  increas- 
ingly complex  organizing  principles. 

Erich  Fromm,  like  Horney,  places  the  causative  factor  of  neurosis 
in  the  family,  which  is  seen  as  the  basic  "agency"  of  enculturation. 
Suppressive  or  hostile  parents  create  the  destructive  feelings  of  power- 
lessness  and  isolation.  Human  relations  and  not  instinctual  pressure 
thus  create  personality.  "Man's  nature,  his  passions,  and  anxieties  are  a 


8  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

cultural  product;  as  a  matter  of  fact  man  himself  is  the  most  important 
creation  and  achievement  of  the  continuous  human  effort,  the  record 
of  which  we  call  history."   (3,  p.  11) 

Fromm's  theories  of  character  are  based  on  the  ways  in  which  the 
individual  "relates"  to  his  world.  He  has  listed  four  neurotic  mech- 
anisms for  "escaping"  insecurity  (masochism,  sadism,  destructiveness, 
and  automaton  conformity)  and  five  character  types  (receptive, 
hoarding,  marketing,  exploitive,  and  productive).  All  of  these  are 
directly  interpersonal.  Fromm's  major  concern  and  greatest  contribu- 
tion lies  not  in  the  area  of  systematization,  but  rather  in  the  philosophic 
backgrounds  he  has  provided  for  the  study  of  personality.  The  nine- 
teenth century  mechanistic  pessimism  of  Freud,  clearly  inadequate  for 
a  science  of  human  nature,  has  received  a  thoughtful,  gentle,  and  imag- 
inative revision  by  Erich  Fromm. 

Harry  Stack  Sullivan's  most  dramatic  accomplishment  was  the 
assertion,  which  I  believe  he  has  demonstrated,  that  "psychiatry  is  the 
study  of  processes  that  involve  or  go  on  between  people.  The  field  of 
psychiatry  is  the  field  of  interpersonal  relations  under  any  and  all 
circumstances  in  which  these  relationships  exist."  (5,  pp.  4-5)  Sul- 
livan's most  valuable  achievement  is  his  demonstration  of  the  "fabu- 
lously more  complicated"  nature  of  interpersonal  actions  and  percep- 
tions, and  the  introduction  of  observational  methods  and  attitudes  for 
making  "objective  contact  with  another  individual." 

The  research  and  the  theories  presented  in  this  book  are  based  on 
the  writings  of  Sullivan,  and  are  in  some  sense  an  attempt  to  extend 
them.  Although  Sullivan's  subtle  and  complex  ideas  do  not  summarize 
readily,  a  brief  survey  is  in  order. 

The  motive  force  of  personality,  for  Sullivan  as  for  Horney  and 
Fromm,  is  the  avoidance  of  anxiety.  Anxiety,  for  all  three,  is  an  inter- 
personal phenomenon.  For  Horney  it  involves  the  feelings  of  help- 
lessness and  danger;  for  Fromm,  isolation  and  weakness;  for  Sullivan, 
loss  of  self-esteem.  Anxiety  is  interpersonal  because  it  is  rooted  in  the 
dreaded  expectation  of  derogation  and  rejection  by  others  (or  by  one- 
self) .  The  human  being  is  rarely  or  never  free  from  some  interpersonal 
tension;  what  he  does  or  thinks  is  generally  related  to  the  estimation 
of  others.  For  this  reason  the  motivating  principle  of  behavior  is  more 
accurately  seen  as  "anxiety  reduction" — the  avoidance  of  the  greater 
nnxiety  and  the  selection  of  the  lesser  anxiety.  This  is  an  important 
point  to  note,  because,  as  we  shall  see  when  we  deal  with  interpersonal 
reflexes,  it  helps  explain  some  of  the  paradoxical  self-punitive  behaviors 
by  means  of  which  individuals  appear  to  make  themselves  unhappy. 

Personality  is,  according  to  Sullivan,  the  "relatively  enduring  pat- 
tern of  recurring  interpersonal  situations  which  characterize  a  human 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY  9 

life."  To  understand  a  person  is  to  have  knowledge  of  the  inter- 
personal techniques  that  he  employs  to  avoid  or  minimize  anxiety  and 
of  the  consistent  pattern  of  relationships  that  he  integrates  as  a  result 
of  these  techniques. 

It  is  important  to  note  that  interpersonal  behavior  refers  to  private 
perceptions,  conscious  reports,  symbolic  and  unwitting  expressions, 
as  well  as  to  overt  actions. 

Another  crucial  difference  between  Sullivan's  conceptions  and  the 
Freudian  is  worth  comment.  According  to  the  orthodox  Freudian, 
that  which  is  warded  off  from  consciousness  is  the  instinctual  impulse 
or  its  disturbing  derivatives.  According  to  Sullivan,  those  things  which 
are  selectively  kept  from  awareness  are  interpersonal  processes,  or 
potentialities,  or  interpersonal  feelings  which  are  anxiety-arousing. 

The  self-dynamism  is  created  by  anxiety,  being  the  system  of 
anxiety-diminishing  behavior  characteristic  of  the  developing  indi- 
vidual. SulUvan  has  distinguished  three  modes  of  experience  which 
have  important  implications:  the  prototaxic,  undifferentiated,  un- 
verbalized  experiences  of  early  infancy;  the  parataxic,  which  includes 
private,  unwitting  personifications  of  the  self  or  eidetic  others;  and 
the  syntaxic.  The  latter  mode  is  defined  by  the  "extent  that  observa- 
tion, analysis,  and  the  eduction  of  relations  is  subjected  to  consensual 
validation  'with  others.'  .  .  ."  Consensual  validation,  a  concept  with 
rich  empirical  meaning,  is  the  "degree  of  approximate  agreement  with 
a  significant  other  person  or  persons  which  permits  fairly  exact  com- 
munication by  speech  or  otherwise,  and  the  drawing  of  generally 
useful  inferences  about  the  action  and  thought  of  the  other."  (6, 
p.  177)  When  two  people  in  an  interaction  situation  are  consensually 
agreed  on  the  basic  premises  upon  which  the  relationship  rests,  and 
when  they  concur  in  their  pertinent  perceptions  of  self  and  each  other, 
then  they  are  communicating  in  the  syntaxic  mode.  This  kind  of 
honesty  between  persons  is  not  a  common  phenomenon.  Its  experi- 
ence can  be  unbearably  painful  due  to  the  anxiety  it  evokes. 

The  discussion  so  far  has  carried  us  with  hazardous  speed  and 
brevity  through  those  conceptions  of  Harry  Stack  Sullivan  which  are 
most  appropriate  to  the  purposes  of  this  volume.  We  leave  without 
any  description  a  host  of  strikingly  original  theories — on  interview 
tactics,  on  obsessional  and  schizophrenic  states,  on  the  six  epochs  of 
personality  development,  on  dissociative  and  selective  inattention,  to 
name  a  few. 

The  weakest  links  in  Sullivan's  strong  conceptual  chain  are  the 
systematic.  His  publications  up  to  the  present  (including  posthumous 
volumes)  have  broken  new  theoretical  ground  that  has  not  been  sown 
or  harvested.   He  presents  an  approach  but  not  a  methodology.   He 


,o  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

convincingly  buries  the  much-berated  remains  of  descriptive,  Kraepe- 
linian,  and  negatively-value-toned  psychiatry,  but  provides  no  sub- 
stitute classification  system.  The  carefully  worked-out  categories  he 
presented — experience  modes,  developmental  epochs,  self-dynamisms 
— are  far  from  the  minimum  required  for  a  science  of  personality. 

Sullivan  provides  an  attitude  (humility)  and  an  approach  (par- 
ticipant observation),  but  not  a  methodology  for  the  science  to  which 
he  was  dedicated.  His  formal  notational  structure  is  disappointingly 
disorganized  and  incomplete. 

The  Theories  of  Erik  H.  Erikson 

In  the  preceding  section  we  have  considered  the  contributions  of 
three  personality  theorists  who  have  abandoned  the  libido  conception 
and  espoused  a  social  or  interpersonal  point  of  view.  Horney,  Fromm, 
and  Sullivan  do  not  deny  the  importance  of  sexual  and  biological 
factors.  Sullivan,  for  example,  divides  human  performance  into  two 
categories  based  on  the  "end  states"  or  goals  which  are  involved.  The 
first  involves  "satisfactions,"  by  which  Sullivan  denotes  bodily  activ- 
ities. The  second  end  state  is  "security,"  which  refers  to  the  inter- 
personal or  cultural  responses.  Having  paid  his  respects  to  the  biologi- 
cal facet  of  human  behavior,  Sullivan  went  on  to  focus  almost 
exclusively  on  security  operations  and  the  social  dimension  of  behavior. 

In  contrast  to  the  antilibido  theorists  mentioned  above,  there  is  a 
fourth  social  system  of  personality  which  attempts  to  develop  ego, 
cultural,  and  interpersonal  conceptions  within  the  basic  framework  of 
the  Freudian  psychosexual  theory.  This  is  the  work  of  Erik  H. 
Erikson.  (1) 

Erikson  includes  in  his  systematic  writings  three  personality  proc- 
esses, the  somatic,  the  ego,  and  the  societal.  He  demonstrates  (by 
means  of  a  brilliant  marshaling  of  clinical  material)  that  a  human 
event  cannot  be  understood  unless  the  relativity  of  these  three  factors 
is  grasped. 

We  study  individual  human  crises  by  becoming  therapeutically  involved  in 
them.  In  doing  so,  we  find  that  the  three  processes  mentioned  are  three  aspects 
of  one  process— i.e.,  human  life,  both  words  being  equally  emphasized.  Somatic 
tension,  individual  anxiety,  and  group  panic,  then,  are  only  different  ways  in 
which  human  anxiety  presents  itself  to  different  methods  of  investigation.  .  .  . 
As  we  review  each  relevant  item  in  a  given  case,  we  cannot  escape  the  convic- 
tion that  the  meaning  of  an  item  which  may  be  "located"  in  one  of  the  three 
processes  is  co-determined  by  its  meaning  in  the  other  two.  An  item  in  one 
process  gains  relevance  by  giving  significance  to  and  receiving  significance  from 
Items  in  the  others.  Gradually,  I  hope,  we  may  find  better  words  for  this 
relativity  in  human  existence— &s  we  shall  tentatively  call  what  we  wish  to 
demonstrate.  (1,  p.  33) 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY  1 1 

Erikson  has  made  the  most  sophisticated  and  successful  attempt  to 
integrate  historical,  sociological,  anthropological,  and  biological  data 
into  a  personality  system.  He  takes  for  his  model  of  individual  char- 
acter structure  the  Freudian  psychosexual  theory  to  which  he  has 
added  an  interpersonal  terminology.  His  commitment  to  the  biology 
of  the  libido  theory  is  stated  quite  directly.  "It  will  seem  to  some  that 
I  am  abandoning  this  point  of  view  [i.e.,  the  importance  of  interper- 
sonal regulation  patterns]  as  I  now  proceed  to  review  the  whole  field 
of  what  Freud  called  pregenital  stages  and  erotogenic  zones  in  child- 
hood and  attempt  to  build  a  bridge  from  clinical  experience  to  observa- 
tions on  societies.  For  I  will  again  speak  of  biologically  given  poten- 
tialities which  develop  with  the  child's  organism.  I  do  not  think  that 
psychoanalysis  can  remain  a  workable  system  of  inquiry  without  its 
basic  biological  formulations,  much  as  they  may  need  reconsidera- 
tion." (l,p.  65) 

Erikson  has  expanded  and  "socialized"  the  Freudian  timetable  of 
psychosexual  adjustment  by  means  of  two  ingenious  systematic 
devices — his  conceptions  of  zones,  modes,  and  modalities  and  his 
theory  of  the  eight  stages  of  man's  psychological  development. 

Erikson  focuses  on  three  major  zones  of  psychosexual  activity — 
oral,  anal,  and  genital.  He  then  defines  five  modes  of  approach  or  basic 
interpersonal  vectors  which  can  be  expressed  by  any  organ  zone. 
These  are  incorporative  1  (sucking),  incorporative  2  (biting),  reten- 
tive, eliminative,  and  mtrusive.  A  matrix  of  the  combination  of  zones 
and  modes  provides  a  neat  device  for  classifying  the  fixations,  regres- 
sions, and  sequences  of  normal  development. 

An  even  more  original  conversion  of  Freudian  developmental 
theory  to  interpersonal  language  is  accomplished  by  Erikson  by  means 
of  his  eight  stages  of  human  emotional  growth.  This  is  a  "list  of  ego 
qualities — criteria  by  which  the  individual  demonstrates  that  his  ego, 
at  a  given  stage,  is  strong  enough  to  integrate  the  timetable  of  the 
organism  with  the  structure  of  social  institutions."  Erikson  holds  that 
the  individual  at  each  sequential  stage  of  life  meets  a  nuclear  conflict, 
the  solution  for  which  "is  based  on  the  integration  of  the  earlier  ones." 

The  eight  nuclear  conflicts  according  to  Erikson  are: 

Stage  of  Life  Cycle  Nuclear  Conflict 

Oral  Sensory  Trust  vs.  Mistrust 

Muscular— Anal  Autonomy  vs.  Shame,  Doubt 

Locomotor— Genital  Initiative  vs.  Guilt 

Latency  Industry  vs.  Inferiority 

Puberty  and  Adolescence  Identity  vs.  Role  Diffusion 

Young  Adulthood  Intimacy  vs.  Isolation 

Adulthood  Generativity  vs.  Stagnation 

Maturity  Integrity  vs.  Disgust,  Despair 


12  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

Erikson's  commitment  to  an  interpersonal  and  cultural  point  of 
view  stands  out  clearly  in  this  list  of  ego  qualities.  The  extraordinary- 
power  and  significance  of  Erikson's  work  is  this:  he  has  developed  a 
social  conception  of  human  nature  which  certainly  equals  in  com- 
plexity those  of  Fromm  and  Horney — and  he  has  done  it  within  the 
broad  framework  of  the  Freudian  libido  theory.  He  seems  to  have 
succeeded  in  his  attempt  to  build  a  bridge  between  psychosexual 
theory  and  social  behavior,  and  has  additionally  erected  a  system 
which  is  eminently  heuristic. 

There  is  therefore  considerable  justification  for  considering  Erikson 
as  the  first  major  psychoanalytic  systematist  since  Freud.  He  has,  it 
must  be  noted,  surpassed  Sullivan  on  his  own  home  ground  by  pre- 
senting a  developmental  timetable  which  lists  sixteen  interpersonal 
resolutions.  This  provides  us  with  an  impressive  list  of  interpersonal 
variables  lacking  in  the  writings  of  the  less  systematic  Sullivan. 

The  interpersonal  system  of  personality  to  be  presented  in  this 
book  has  leaned  heavily  upon  the  conceptions  of  Erik  H.  Erikson.  Our 
classification  of  interpersonal  behavior  bears  the  unmistakable  mark  of 
Erikson's  theory.  We  have  been  able  to  utilize  only  a  fragment  of  his 
system.  This  is  because  Erikson's  writings  range  deep  and  wide — deep 
into  childhood  and  wide  into  society.  Our  own  purpose  and  efforts 
are  much  more  restricted  since  we  have  attempted  simply  to  develop 
an  objective,  functional  system  for  predicting  the  behavior  of  adult 
patients  in  the  psychiatric  clinic. 

Interpersonal  Behavior  Defines  the  Most  Important 
Dimension  of  Personality 

In  the  preceding  sections  we  have  presented  a  definition  of  inter- 
personal behavior  and  have  compared  several  approaches  to  human 
nature  in  the  light  of  their  social  orientations.  The  assertion  was  made 
that  the  interpersonal  can  from  this  point  of  view  be  considered  the 
most  crucial  and  functionally  important  dimension  of  personality. 

First,  from  the  broader  theoretical  frame  of  reference,  interpersonal 
behavior  is  crucial  to  the  survival  of  the  human  being.  From  a  second 
(and  much  more  parochial)  point  of  view,  interpersonal  behavior  is 
the  aspect  of  personality  that  is  most  functionally  relevant  to  the 
clinician.  Some  justification  for  the  first  of  these  assertions  will  be 
discussed  in  the  next  section.  The  usefulness  of  an  interpersonal 
theory  in  clinical  practice  will  be  considered  in  Chapter  5. 

Interpersonal  Behavior  and  Biological  Survival 

From  the  standpoint  of  human  survival,  social  role  and  social  ad- 
justment comprise  the  most  important  dimension  of  personality.  This 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY  i  3 

is  because  of  the  unique  biological  and  cultural  aspects  of  human  devel- 
opment and  maturity. 

One  of  the  major  differences  between  man  and  the  other  animal 
species  is  his  long  and  helpless  infancy.  Depending  on  the  complexity 
of  the  culture,  it  takes  from  12  to  25  years  for  a  human  being  to  attain 
developmental  maturity.  This  long  period  of  childhood  and  adoles- 
cence involves  a  dependence  on  other  human  beings  for  nourishment, 
shelter,  and  security.  Many  animal  species,  on  the  contrary,  are  ready 
to  undertake  complete  responsibility  for  their  own  survival  at  birth, 
or  shortly  thereafter.  In  these  cases  instinctual  methods  of  locomotion, 
food  collection,  and  self-protection  take  over  immediately.  Rigidly 
built-in  patterns  of  response  are  vital  to  their  early  self-sufficiency. 
Automatic  physiological  responses  are  the  key  to  life  for  these  infra- 
human  organisms. 

The  case  of  man  is  quite  different.  The  human  infant  has  limited 
physical  capacity  and  few  automatic  behavior  sequences  for  dealing 
directly  with  the  physical  environment.  From  the  moment  of  birth, 
survival  depends  on  the  adequacy  of  interpersonal  relationships.  The 
water,  warmth,  and  milk  upon  which  the  infant's  life  depends  come 
from  others.  These  primitive,  basic  transactions  which  the  neonate 
carries  on  with  others  are,  we  are  told,  not  rigidly  fixed  patterns.  A 
variety  of  early  parental  response  exists,  and  this  is  matched  by  a  varia- 
tion in  neonate  behavior.  Several  experts  in  this  field  (Sullivan,  Klein, 
Erikson,  Ribble,  Spitz)  have  claimed  that  the  roots  of  personality  are 
to  be  found  in  the  earliest  mother-child  interactions.  This  claim  is  not 
surprising  when  we  recall  that  a  raw,  intense,  basic  anxiety  (concerned 
with  the  maintenance  of  life  itself)  may  be  felt  by  the  neonate.  And 
this  anxiety  is  dealt  with  (partially  or  completely,  carelessly  or  lov- 
ingly, calmly  or  nervously)  by  the  mothering-one.  The  earliest 
kind  of  survival  anxiety  is,  therefore,  handled  by  interpersonal,  social 
responses. 

From  the  standpoint  of  physiology  the  human  infant  is  not  much 
different  from  any  young  mammal.  From  the  standpoint  of  per- 
sonahty  psychology,  however,  the  human  being  at  birth  is  an  extraor- 
dinarily plastic,  germinal  nucleus  with  infinite  potentialities  for 
eventual  differentiation.  It  might  be  said  that  any  neonate  is  a  potential 
president,  priest,  poet,  or  psychotic.  PersonaUty  psychology  is  con- 
cerned with  the  events  and  behaviors  which  determine  the  emotional 
and  social  development  of  the  individual.  The  most  important  factors 
which  account  for  the  wide  varieties  of  behavior  characteristic  of  the 
human  being  are  the  interpersonal  security  operations  which  he 
develops  and  the  social  relationships  (real  and  fantasied)  which  he 
integrates  with  others. 


14  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

We  have  pointed  to  the  crucial  influence  of  the  earliest  social  trans- 
actions between  mother  and  child — crucial  because  of  the  survival 
anxiety  involved  and  because  of  the  complete  dependence  of  the 
infant. 

As  the  child  grows,  the  primacy  of  interpersonal  relationships  does 
not  lessen  greatly.  A  seven-year-old  child  has  developed  many  motoric 
patterns  for  self-protection,  but  on  the  hypothetical  desert  island  or  in 
any  societal  context  we  cannot  credit  him  with  survival  self- 
sufficiency. 

The  human  being  maintains  existence  by  virtue  of  the  long  period 
of  parental  protection  during  which  he  assimilates  the  complicated 
cultural  wisdom  necessary  for  survival.  This  process  of  slow,  and 
often  painful,  learning  is  intensely  interpersonal. 

Even  at  maturity  survival  rests  upon  successful  interpersonal  pat- 
terns. The  mutual  dependence  of  mankind  is  inevitable.  Whether  we 
exist  in  a  primitive  tribe,  a  dictatorship,  or  an  industrial  democracy, 
the  key  to  human  life  lies  in  the  adequacy  of  social  interaction.  Even 
the  rare  test  case  of  a  hermit  falls  within  the  limits  of  this  generaliza- 
tion, since  this  adjustment  technique  always  involves  intense  and  often 
bitter  "withdrawal"  from  others,  and  is  one  pattern  of  interpersonal 
reactivity.  The  extent  to  which  we  autoniatically  and  implicitly 
demonstrate  patterns  of  cooperation  and  submission  to  social  demands 
— even  in  the  most  democratic  society — is  quite  striking.  Failure  to  do 
so  invites  such  real  or  fantasied  threats  to  life  that  we  automatically 
commit  ourselves  in  countless  ways  to  the  interpersonal  pressure  of 
parents,  societies,  and  contemporaries. 

Anxiety  Motivates  Interpersonal  Behavior 

The  preceding  section  is  intended  to  justify  the  statement  that  inter- 
personal behavior  has  a  basic  survival  function.  The  fear  of  inter- 
personal disaster  is  rooted  in  a  fear  of  destruction  or  abandonment. 
The  organism  has  hundreds  of  physiological  functions  by  which  de- 
struction is  warded  off  and  life  preserved.  The  individual  develops, 
in  addition,  numerous  emotional  responses  which,  in  their  origins,  are 
concerned  with  survival. . 

The  psychological  expression  of  the  survival  drive  of  evolution 
theory  is  anxiety.  Primal  anxiety  is  the  fear  of  abandonment.^  As  the 
child  begins  to  develop,  this  becomes  a  fear  of  rejection  and  social 
disapproval.    Mankind's  social  interdependence  means  that  extreme 

*  In  the  first  version  of  this  manuscript  this  sentence  read,  "Primal  anxiety  is  the 
fear  of  death."  The  revision  vi^as  made  at  the  suggestion  of  Harvey  Powelson,  M.D., 
who  pointed  out  that  death  is  a  sophisticated,  complex  concept  which  an  infant  or 
young  child  has  not  mastered. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION  OF  PERSONALITY 


15 


derogation  on  the  part  of  crucial  others  can  lead  to  destruction.  The 
behaviors  by  which  the  child  avoids  derogation  are  called  security 
operations.  They  assure  him  of  the  approval  and  social  security  which 
reduce  his  anxiety. 

As  the  individual  develops,  further  complications  ensue.  Self- 
esteem  becomes  a  factor  which  is  equal  to,  or  greater  than,  the  overt 
esteem  of  others.^ 

The  role  of  anxiety  in  the  development  of  human  personality  is 
central,  and  it  is  intricate  beyond  our  understanding.  Although  rooted 
genetically  in  the  fear  of  death,  anxiety  (i.e.,  the  fear  of  disapproval) 
is  clearly  stronger  in  the  case  of  the  adult  than  the  fear  of  death.  There 
are  countless  examples  of  human  beings  choosing  to  face  and  accept 
destruction  rather  than  face  anxiety  and  the  loss  of  self-esteem. 
Suicide  is  one  of  many  such  examples. 

Another  complication  which  must  be  considered  in  understanding 
the  effects  of  anxiety  involves  the  multilevel  organization  of  behaviors 
for  warding  off  anxiety.  A  large  percentage  of  any  population,  for 
example,  develops  security  operations  which  entail  overt  self-efface- 
ment, self-derogation,  and  the  provocation  of  actual  contempt  and 
disapproval  from  others.  These  overt  self-derogations,  which  seem  to 
contradict  our  theory  of  anxiety,  can  be  understood  by  means  of  a 
multilevel  analysis.  They  are  inevitably  related  to  private  feelings  of 
uniqueness  or  secret  consolations.  They  serve  to  protect  inner  feelings 
of  pride  and  self-enhancement. 

This  book  and  the  system  of  personality  which  it  describes  is  con- 
cerned with  a  multilevel  investigation  of  human  security  operations. 
We  have  taken  as  our  task  the  definition,  classification,  and  measure- 
ment of  interpersonal  behavior  (at  several  levels).  We  view  the  inter- 
personal behavior  of  an  individual  as  the  machinery  by  means  of  which 
he  wards  off  anxiety  and  maintains  a  multilevel  balance  of  self- 
enhancement. 

The  conceptual  model  of  personality  which  we  are  developing 
exposes  one  area  of  human  behavior  to  study.  This  is  the  interpersonal 
dimension.  The  theoretical  system  is  based  on  one  assumption  about 
the  motivation  of  emotional  behavior.  This  has  been  formalized  as 
follows: 

First  working  principle:  Personality  is  the  multilevel  pattern  of 
interpersonal  responses  (overt,  conscious,  or  private)  expressed  by  the 
individual.   Interpersonal  behavior  is  aimed  at  reducing  anxiety.   All 

^The  complexity  of  the  processes  of  identification  and  introj action  make  this  com- 
parison redundant  and  probably  meaningless.  There  is  good  reason  to  believe  that  self- 
esteem  is  usually  or  always  based  on  values  which  are  taken  from  others.  Thus  self- 
esteem  can  be  considered  an  indirect  form  of  approval  of  crucial  others. 


1 6  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

the  social,  emotional,  interpersonal  activities  of  an  individual  can  be 
understood  as  attempts  to  avoid  anxiety  or  to  establish  and  maintain 
self-esteem. 

References 

1.  Erikson,  E.  H.  Childhood  and  society.  New  York:  Norton,  1950. 

2.  Fenichel,  O.   Psychoanalytic  theory  of  neurosis.   New  York:  Norton,  1945. 

3.  Fromm,  E.   Escape  from  freedom.   New  York:  Rinehan,  1947. 

4.  HoRNEY,  Karen.    Our  inner  conflicts.   New  York:  Norton,  1945. 

5.  Sullivan,  H.  S.  Conceptions  of  modem  psychiatry.  Washington,  D.C.:  The  Wil- 
liam Alanson  White  Psychiatric  Foundation,  1947. 

6.  SuLUVAN,  H.  S.  Multidisciplined  coordination  of  interpersonal  data.  In  S.  S 
Sargent  and  Marian  W.  Smith  (eds.).  Culture  and  personality.  New  York: 
Viking  Fund,  1949. 


Adjustment-Maladjustment  Factors  in 
Personality  Theory 


It  is  the  theme  of  this  chapter  that  personality  theories  should  hold  for 
adjustive  and  maladjustive  behaviors,  that  normality  and  abnormality 
should  be  defined  as  different  points  on  the  same  measurement  con- 
tinuum, and  that  the  conceptual  terminology  of  personality  should 
therefore  include  the  entire  adjustive  range  of  human  activity.  Few 
theories  do  this.  Most  are  oriented  toward  abnormal  or  neurotic 
behaviors.  Most  diagnostic  systems  have  few  terms  for  conceptualiz- 
ing adaptive  behavior,  which  is  described  in  vague  generalities  or  in 
terms  of  the  absence  of  pathology. 

This  is  an  unfortunate  state  of  affairs.  It  reflects  an  undeliberate 
but  significant  depreciation  of  human  nature.  In  addition,  this  pathol- 
ogy error  tends  to  distort  our  theories  of  personality  by  placing  a 
disproportionate  emphasis  on  certain  limited  types  of  maladjustment, 

A  science  of  malfunction  cannot  precede  a  science  of  function. 
Therapeutic  tactics  can  break  new  ground,  but  scientific  and  theoretic 
progress  depends  upon  the  development  of  the  principles  of  normal 
adjustment.  The  fact  that  psychiatric  theories  of  personality  have  been 
based  on  clinical  experiences  has  led  to  some  curiously  one-sided 
conceptualizations.  Psychiatry,  however,  cannot  be  wholly  blamed 
for  these  restrictions,  which,  as  we  shall  see,  spring  from  a  marked 
asymmetry  in  the  ethical  evaluations  of  varying  interpersonal  themes 
in  our  Western  culture. 

Before  approaching  the  definitions  of  adjustment-maladjustment 
we  shall  review  psychiatry's  overemphasis  on  the  abnormal,  and  we 
shall  consider  some  causes  and  implications  of  this  pathology  error. 

Psychiatric  Theories  Are  Oriented  Towards  Pathology 

A  history  of  man's  conception  of  his  own  nature  has  yet  to  be 
written.   When  our  systematic  knowledge  of  human  expressive  be- 

'7 


1 8  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

havior  is  more  advanced,  it  will  be  possible  to  study  the  literary  and 
historical  documents  of  the  past,  and  to  determine  the  expressed  and 
implied  views  of  personality  that  determined  the  behavior  of  our 
ancestors.  One  tentative  generalization — basic  to  the  theory  of  this 
book — may  be  helpful  in  surveying  the  changing  conceptions  of 
human  nature.  This  concerns  the  Locus  of  Responsibility  for  human 
behavior.  There  seems  to  be  a  consistent  tendency  in  the  development 
of  psychological  knowledge  to  move  the  causative  factor  of  human 
behavior  from  external  to  internal  forces.  This  is  clearly  reflected  in 
the  changes  in  the  theoretical  explanations  of  abnormal  or  maladjus- 
tive  behavior. 

We  are  told  that  success  or  failure  appeared,  to  the  ancients,  to  be 
controlled  by  the  immutable  and  mysterious  powers  of  nature.  Sun, 
seed,  and  storm  were  fearful  forces — completely  inexplicable.  Man's 
survival  responses  appeared  by  comparison  quite  meaningless.  The 
shift  of  causative  principles  to  anthropomorphic  gods  made  human 
behavior  somewhat  more  important.  The  notion  that  man  can  move 
the  gods  by  propitiation,  obedience,  or  defiance  considerably  human- 
izes the  causative  sequence. 

This  conception  which  lasted  from  the  Greek  civilization  through 
to  the  nineteenth  century  (and  which  still  is  maintained  by  a  large 
majority  of  individuals  living  today)  defines  personality  aberration  as  a 
religious  phenomenon.  Maladjustment  is  a  mark  of  omnipotent  inter- 
vention, generally  indicating  a  sinful  nature.  The  maladjusted  person 
is  isolated,  overtly  punished,  or  covertly  rejected.  The  error  is  man's 
and  the  power  is  the  god's. 

The  theories  of  descriptive  psychiatry  which  emphasized  constitu- 
tional morbidity,  although  they  had  the  ring  of  scientific  objectivity, 
were  still  very  crude  conceptions.  They  were  abysmally  inferior 
to  the  insights  of  the  artistic  geniuses  who  preceded  them  by  several 
centuries.  Shakespeare,  for  example,  progressed  much  further  from 
the  Greek  mythology  than  the  average  hospital  psychiatrist  of  the 
early  1900's.  On  the  other  hand,  in  the  strictest  sense  of  dramatic 
motivation,  Oedipus  was  a  morbidly  predisposed  type — since  no 
choice  is  given  him  at  any  point  to  reverse  his  awful  destiny.  This 
type  of  psychological  explanation  is  quite  congenial  to  pre-Freudian 
psychiatry.  Change  a  few  mythological  terms  and  Oedipus  is  an 
acceptable  case  history  from  the  textbook  of  the  nineteenth  century 
alienist.  When  we  compare  this  predestined  helplessness  with  the 
self-imposed  conflicts  of  Shakespeare's  characters  the  descriptive 
psychiatrist  comes  off  badly.  Although  Elizabethan  theories  of  human 
destiny  involved  chance  and  fortuitous  influences  (the  wheel  of  fate), 
still  the  reader  is  impressed  by  the  implication  that  the  poet's  heroes 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  19 

court  their  tragic  ends  because  of  their  own  greed,  ambition,  indeci- 
sion, and  shallowness.  The  causative  agency  has  moved  from  the  ex- 
ternal and  immovable  force  to  the  partial  responsibility  of  the  hero  for 
his  own  self-created  destiny. 

The  notion  that  human  nature  and  the  individual's  fate  are  deter- 
mined by  his  own  (conscious  or  unconscious)  decisions  and  solutions 
is  brilliantly  illustrated  by  Marcel  Proust.  In  Remembrance  of  Things 
Past  he  describes  how  his  hero  deliberately  trains  and  provokes  his 
parents  to  accept  him  as  a  neurotic  child.  In  the  following  episode  he 
literally  creates  his  own  maladjustment  and  develops  the  weak  and 
asocial  role  he  is  to  maintain  in  his  future  life.  His  parents  agree, 
"  'It  is  his  nerves  .  .  .'  .  And  thus  for  the  first  time  my  unhappiness 
was  regarded  no  longer  as  a  fault  for  which  I  must  be  punished,  but  as 
an  involuntary  evil  which  has  been  officially  recognized,  a  nervous 
condition  for  which  I  was  in  no  way  responsible:  I  had  the  consola- 
tion that  I  need  no  longer  mingle  apprehensive  scruples  with  the  bit- 
terness of  my  tears;  I  could  weep  henceforth  without  sin." 

The  narrator  recognizes,  however,  that  his  neurosis  is  not  "involun- 
tary," but  rather  a  purposive,  victorious  interpersonal  maneuver.  He 
has  unconsciously  selected  nervousness  as  a  security  operation.  The 
narrator  then  goes  on  to  say,  "I  ought  then  to  be  happy;  I  was  not.  It 
struck  me  that  my  mother  had  just  made  a  first  concession  which  must 
have  been  painful  to  her,  that  it  was  a  first  step  down  from  the  ideal 
she  had  formed  for  me,  and  that  for  the  first  time  she,  with  all  her 
courage,  had  to  confess  herself  beaten.  It  struck  me  that  if  I  had  just 
scored  a  victory  it  was  over  her;  that  I  had  succeeded,  as  sickness  or 
sorrow  or  age  might  have  succeeded,  in  relaxing  her  will,  in  altering 
her  judgment;  that  this  evening  opened  a  new  era,  must  remain  a  black 
date  in  the  calendar."   (4,  p.  49) 

Freudian  Theory  of  Normality 

With  the  Freudian  theory,  psychology  begins  to  catch  up  with 
the  intuitions  of  literature.  Man's  character,  his  responses  and  solu- 
tions to  the  overwhelming  conflicts  of  life  are  brought  into  focus. 
While  man  has  a  choice  of  reactions  which  bring  relative  amounts  of 
temporary  security,  the  balance,  according  to  Freud,  is  still  on  the  side 
of  the  native,  instinctual  endowment.  The  doctrine  of  instincts  em- 
phasizes the  inevitable  pressure  of  drives  external  to  the  ego.  In  early 
psychoanalytic  theory  it  is  libidinal  drive  that  is  basic,  inborn,  con- 
stant, and,  in  the  final  sense,  victorious.  The  adaptive  forces  are 
acquired,  inconstant,  variable,  and,  in  the  final  sense,  secondary.  In 
fact,  the  ego  functions,  defense  mechanisms,  and  character  traits  were 
sometimes  interpreted  as  neurotic  solutions. 


20  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

By  building  his  logical  notational  structure  on  the  "id"  instincts, 
Freud  was  making  a  formal  decision,  and  not  an  empirical  discovery. 
Impressed  by  the  new  insights  he  obtained  into  the  antisocial  impulses, 
it  was  natural  for  Freud  to  base  his  theory  on  that  aspect  of  human 
motivation.  It  is  generally  accepted  that  later  developments  in  psycho- 
analysis have  reversed  this  trend,  and  have  placed  more  emphasis  on 
the  ego,  studying  its  structure,  function,  and  the  multiplex  variety  of 
its  processes.  But  it  is  also  commonly  known  that  early  psychoanalytic 
terminology  tends  to  lack  terms  for  describing  adjustive  behavior  and 
normal  processes.  The  conceptual  contributions  of  Erikson  have 
competently  filled  in  this  gap  in  the  psychoanalytic  nosology. 

The  psychoanalytic  theory  of  personality,  which  is  by  far  the 
most  complete  and  complex  theory,  is  based  on  the  statistically  narrow, 
neurotic  extreme  of  the  general  population  in  two  or  three  Occidental 
countries.  As  we  shall  see  subsequently,  there  is  good  evidence  to 
suggest  that  early  psychiatric  and  psychoanalytic  theory  was  based 
on  less  than  one  half  of  the  range  of  this  maladjustive  extreme,  and 
that  perhaps  50  per  cent  of  neurotic  solutions  remained  largely  un- 
defined. 

The  curious  phenomenon  of  a  massive  theoretical  structure  erected 
on  an  emaciated  sample  of  subjects  is,  I  believe,  due  to  two  basic 
factors,  one  logical  and  one  empirical.  Freud's  formal  choice  in  em- 
phasizing the  destructive  strivings  is  historically  comprehensible,  and 
no  detraction  from  his  creative  genius. 

The  empirical  factor,  as  I  have  suggested,  refers  to  the  narrow 
range  of  individuals  whose  neurosis  is  such  as  to  lead  them  to  submit 
to  the  singular  and  rather  implausible  process  of  psychoanalysis  (cf. 
Chapter  12). 

Jung's  Emphasis  on  Adaptive  Behavior 

The  Jungian  school  of  analytic  psychology  produced  several  im- 
portant revisions  of  Freudian  concepts.  Most  of  its  unique  contribu- 
tions are  refinements  and  extensions  of  Freudian  theory.  To  the  extent 
that  any  cognitive  issue  was  involved,  we  can  say  that  the  Zurich 
group  split  off  from  Vienna  when  Jung  rejected  the  narrow  sexual 
interpretation  of  libidinal  energy.  By  broadening  the  meaning  of  this 
basic  impulse,  Jung  and  his  followers  have  made  it  general  and  vague, 
and  thus  relegated  it  to  a  secondary  theoretical  position.  This  indirect 
shelving  of  the  libido  theory  can  be  taken  as  an  unpremeditated,  but 
vital,  aspect  of  the  Jungian  position.  Other  revisions  pertinent  to  this 
discussion  include  theories  of  functions,  neurosis,  and  unconscious 
motivation. 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  2i 

The  Jungian  functions — extroversion,  intuition,  thinking,  etc. — are 
seen  as  important,  but  not  necessarily  negative,  psychological  mech- 
anisms. They  are  pathological  only  when  rigidly  misused  or  when 
completely  repressed.  For  the  most  part,  when  a  Jungian  diagnostician 
calls  the  subject  "introverted"  he  is  not  making  a  value  judgment;  he 
implies  only  that  this  is  an  important  way  in  which  the  patient  handles 
experience  and  its  conflicts,  and  it  may  or  may  not  be  necessary  to 
modify  its  use. 

It  follows,  then,  that  the  Jungians  do  not  see  character  distortions  as 
pathological  fixations  or  regressions  to  inevitable  infantile  stages.  They 
describe  neurosis  as  a  partial  solution  to  life's  dilemmas — a  construc- 
tive mobilizing  of  "psychic"  resources  against  real  or  imagined  threats. 
They  might  say  of  the  neurotic  pattern,  "This  is  a  good  try,  perhaps 
the  best  you  could  do  under  those  circumstances.  Now  let's  see  what 
the  results  of  these  solutions  have  been  and  what  other  possibilities  for 
resolution  we  can  discover." 

This  approach  has  much  to  recommend  it.  It  is  very  congenial  to 
the  current  medical  conception  which  defines  disease,  not  as  an  un- 
fortunate falling  ill,  but  as  a  complicated  interaction  between  one  net- 
work of  adaptive  responses  and  another  network  of  threatening  events. 

Another,  and  perhaps  the  greatest,  advantage  of  the  Jungian  system 
is  the  conception  of  unconscious  motivations  as  valuable,  undiscovered 
potentials  of  the  self,  rather  than  as  destructive  impulses.  Bateson  has 
appraised  the  Jungian  viewpoint  as  more  consistent  with  the  prin- 
ciples of  communication  theory.  He  points  out  that 

the  Freudian  ambition  to  substitute  ego  for  id  or  to  include  the  id  within  the 
scope  of  the  ego,  sounds  to  Jungians  like  advocating  manipulative  and  conscious 
control  of  the  foreign  body.  In  reply  to  this  they  would  urge  merely  the 
acceptance— even  the  joyful  acceptance— of  the  fact  that  the  foreign  body 
though  always  and  inevitably  unconscious  is  really  a  part  of  the  self  and  the  self 
a  part  of  it— the  collective  unconscious  being  imagined  to  be  in  some  sense 
greater  than  the  self.  (5,  p.  264) 

With  this  background  it  becomes  clear  that  the  Jungian  theory, 
although  based  on  and  indebted  to  the  work  of  Freud,  has  made  certain 
advances  toward  a  balanced  conception  of  normahty-abnormality 
factors.  Shifting  the  stress  from  infantile  strivings  to  the  selecting  and 
adapting  functions  of  response  helps  to  free  psychology  from  fatalistic 
themes  which  have  limited  man's  view  of  human  nature  from 
Sophocles'  time  through  Freud's. 

Jungian  theories  have  contributed,  often  indirectly,  to  four  promis- 
ing notions.  First  they  bring  us  closer  to  the  development  of  a 
normality-abnormality  continuum,  which  makes  neurosis  not  a  quali- 


22  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

tatively  different  phenomenon.  They  help  us  see  the  interaction  be- 
tween biological-cultural  pressures  and  the  adaptive-maladaptive 
responses  of  the  individual.  They  emphasize  the  "circular  or  reticu- 
late" equilibrium  of  different  levels  of  personality  rather  than  the 
one-sided  organization  for  warding  off  unconscious  motivations. 
Finally,  they  are,  perhaps,  the  first  to  introduce  the  far-reaching  idea 
that  unconscious  or  repressed  motives  can  be  positive,  constructive 
potentials,  and  are  not  necessarily  negative. 

Many  of  these  doctrines  were  only  implicit  in  Jung's  writings,  and 
credit  for  their  informal,  undramatic  development  must  be  assigned 
to  certain  American  analytic  psychiatrists,  in  particular,  Joseph  Wheel- 
wright and  Joseph  Henderson. 

Homey  and  Fromm  on  Normality 

This  general  tendency  to  focus  upon  adjustive  behavior  has  been 
given  articulate  expression  by  psychiatric  systemists  who  have  em- 
phasized the  cultural  dimension  of  personality.  When  Horney  and 
Fromm  substitute  cultural  factors  for  instinctual  pressure  in  the 
causative  formula,  they  bring  about  drastic  revision  in  attitudes 
toward  mental  health  and  disease.  In  the  first  place,  the  sexual  and 
aggressive  instincts — defined  by  Freud  as  universal,  immutable,  and 
antisocial — tend  to  taint  all  men  with  a  new  form  of  original  sin.  The 
culture  concept  is  much  more  flexible.  It  gives  man,  or  sohie  men,  a 
halfway  chance  because  of  the  wide  variation  in  social  environments 
and  cultural  pressures. 

Thus  the  diagnostician's  causative  questions  become:  "What  were 
the  set  of  biological,  familial,  social,  and  cultural  pressures  which  this 
patient  faced,  and  what  was  the  particular  network  of  responses  by 
which  he  dealt  with  them?"  The  issue  of  normality-abnormality  takes 
on  new  meaning  in  this  context.  A  survey  of  the  publications  of 
Fromm  or  Horney  will  reveal  the  extent  to  which  these  authors  are 
concerned  with  the  individual's  attempts  to  solve  his  conflicts.  We 
have  in  the  previous  chapter  cited  a  partial  list  of  some  dozen  mech- 
anisms, escapes,  and  trends  described  by  these  two  theorists.  Over 
and  over  again  they  emphasize  the  response  of  the  patient  to  the  en- 
vironment, and  his  interactions  with  it.  Their  interest  in  pathology  is 
always  hnked  to  the  underlying  notion  that  neurosis  is  acquired  by 
and  through  the  individual's  reactions  to  social  stress,  and  the  sub- 
sidiary idea  that  it  can  be  "cured"  by  shifting  one's  reactions  to  stress 
in  the  future. 

Basic  and  implicit  to  the  theories  of  both  is  the  theme  that  mal- 
adjustment is  different  in  degree,  and  not  in  kind,  from  the  so-called 
norm.  Fromm  states  this  clearly, 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  23 

The  phenomena  which  we  observe  in  the  neurotic  person  are  in  principle  not 
different  from  those  we  find  in  the  normal.  They  are  only  more  accentuated, 
clear-cut,  and  frequently  more  accessible  to  the  awareness  of  the  neurotic  person 
than  they  are  in  the  normal  who  is  not  aware  of  any  personal  problem  which 
warrants  study.  (1,  p.  17) 

Sullivan  and  the  Concept  of  Normality 

Within  the  framework  of  a  brief  historical  review  we  have  been 
selecting  several  themes  which  comprise  the  message  of  this  chapter. 
These  include  the  qualitative  similarity  of  normality-abnormality,  the 
locus  of  responsibility  assigned  to  the  individual's  behavior,  rather  than 
to  fatalistic  forces,  and  the  necessity  to  take  into  account  the  multi- 
level nature  of  human  potentialities.  These  concepts,  which  are  im- 
plicit in  the  development  of  psychoanalytic  theory  during  the  last 
fifty  years,  appear  over  and  over  again  in  the  writings  of  Sullivan. 
This  theorist,  we  recall,  holds  that  the  self  is  formed  through  the 
child's  sensitivity  to  approval  and  disapproval.  If  we  accept  this  notion 
that  personality  is  determined  by  interpersonal  anxiety  we  have  closed 
the  qualitative  gap  between  normal  and  abnormal.  ''Everything  that 
can  be  found  in  mental  disorder  can  be  found  in  anyone,  but  the 
accent,  the  prominence,  the  misuse,  of  that  which  is  found  in  the 
mental  patient,  is  more  or  less  characteristic."  (3,  p.  77)  With  this  re- 
mark SuUivan  advances  the  concept  of  the  continuity  of  normal 
and  abnormal  human  behavior  which  developed  from  the  orig- 
inal Jungian  protest.  Listing  neurotic  and  normal  behavior  along  a 
relativistic  continuum  is  a  humanistic  trend,  which  results  in  changing 
techniques  in  psychotherapy.  Moreover,  it  lends  itself  more  directly 
to  scientific  procedures,  since  probability  laws  become  considerably 
more  feasible.  The  pathology  error  in  psychiatric  thinking  led  to 
theorems  that  were  based  on  neurotic  behavior,  and  which  had  little 
to  say  about  normal  functioning.  By  concentrating  on  the  processes  of 
adaptation  in  their  successful  and  unsuccessful  forms  the  stage  is  set  for 
many  new  personality  systems  which  will  hold  for  all  human  behavior. 

Emphasis  on  Adaptive  Responses  Leads 

to  a  Neutral  Conceptio?i  of  Human  Nature 

To  insist  that  psychology  focus  on  man's  executive,  adaptive  reac- 
tions— in  their  adjustive  flexibility  as  well  as  their  maladjustive 
extreme — is  not  to  argue  for  a  bright-eyed  optimistic  view  of  the 
human  situation.  In  many  ways  it  is  much  kinder  to  inform  a  fellow 
human  being  that  his  misery  or  failure  is  due  to  divine  direction, 
inherited  disposition,  or  biological  destiny.  We  remember  that 
Sophocles,  while  plunging  Oedipus  into  the  depth  of  despair,  never 
forced  him  to  express  man's  most  poignant  lament,  "I  could  have  done 


J .  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

differently."  His  fate  was  always  in  the  hands  of  the  gods.  The 
responsibility  for  human  destiny  is  thereby  transferred  to  external 
forces.  This  view  relieves  man  of  the  obligation  to  effect  change, 
which  is  assigned  to  omnipotent  powers  among  whom  later  generations 
have  included  the  physician.  This  is  probably  the  easiest  and  most 
comfortable  conception  of  human  nature. 

When  we  interpret  adjustment  in  terms  of  the  individual's  own 
responses,  rigid  solutions,  and  escape  mechanisms,  we  present  our 
fellow-sufferers  with  an  ambiguous  gift.  Two  rather  staggering  im- 
plications accompany  this  conception.  Neither  are  particularly  optim- 
istic. The  first  is,  "You  must  accept  the  blame  or  credit  for  your 
present  situation;  you,  and  not  your  rejecting  parents,  your  race, 
your  instinctual  heritage,  your  drunken  husband,  but  your  own  pat- 
tern of  repetitive  and  self-limiting  responses  created  it."  To  this  grim 
frankness  we  must  add  the  corollary,  "To  you,  therefore,  is  given  the 
power  to  change  your  situation.  .  .  .  it  is  impossible  and  unnecessary 
to  change  your  childhood,  the  society  in  which  you  live,  your  skin 
color,  your  biological  make-up,  or  your  spouse — what  is  required  is  a 
change  in  your  inaccurate  perceptions  and  rigid  reactions." 

When  we  replace  immutable  external  forces  with  self-determinism, 
we  invite  the  individual  to  accept  a  most  lonely  and  frightening 
power  which,  as  Fromm  has  pointed  out,  none  of  us  are  well  trained 
to  assume.  This  is,  of  course,  neither  an  optimistic  nor  a  pessimistic 
point  of  view,  being  rather  the  neutral  realistic  statement  of  the 
reciprocal  principles  of  social  interaction  and  self-determination. 

Symptom  and  Character 

The  changing  approaches  to  personality  just  described  have  re- 
sulted in  an  additional  clarifying  abstraction  which  is  very  pertinent  to 
the  conception  of  neurosis.  This  is  the  distinction  between  symptom 
and  character. 

As  used  in  this  context,  the  term  character  refers  to  the  personality 
— the  durable,  multiple-level  pattern  of  interpersonal  tendencies  or- 
ganized into  stable  or  unstable  equilibria.  This  complex  organization 
of  perception  and  action  is  a  logical  notational  structure  by  which  we 
conceptualize  the  anxiety-reducing  operations  of  the  individual.  It  is 
the  theoretical  and  linguistic  device  by  which  we  summarize  our 
knowledge  of  a  human  being.  The  character  structure,  as  the  sum 
total  of  an  individual's  interpersonal  behavior,  is  the  psychologist's 
shorthand  for  the  social  human  being. 

A  symptom,  as  succinctly  defined  by  Masserman  (2,  p.  298),  is 
any  "overt  manifestation  of  a  disease  or  behavior  disorder."  It  is  one 
aspect  of  the  unified  network  of  variables  that  make  up  personality. 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  25 

and  an  important  aspect  in  that  it  indicates  an  imbalance  or  malfunc- 
tion in  the  character  structure.  A  symptom  not  only  tells  us  that 
something  is  distorted  in  the  personality,  but  in  the  nature  of  its 
specificity  often  suggests  what  kind  of  a  distortion  exists.  Regardless 
of  how  centrally  painful  psychiatric  symptoms  may  be  to  the  patient 
or  to  his  intimates,  their  meaning,  function,  and  treatment  must  be 
viewed  as  one  set  of  factors  related  to  many  others  in  the  personality 
organization. 

To  illustrate  the  distinction  between  symptom  and  character,  let 
us  pose  the  question,  what  do  we  mean  by  neurosis?  Psychiatric  text- 
books define  neurosis  in  terms  of  repetitive,  anxiety-driven  behavior 
based  on  internal  conflict,  and  manifesting  certain  symptomatic  ex- 
pressions. This  is  a  broad,  inclusive,  dictionary-type  definition,  and  a 
pretty  good  one.  It  emphasizes  not  only  the  external  appearance  of 
neurosis — the  symptoms — but  also  the  underlying  character  distor- 
tions. Unhappily  when  the  nonanalytic  psychiatrist  takes  off  his 
Sunday-best  terminology  and  lists  his  workday  operating  diagnostic 
concepts,  this  nice  balance  is  lost.  Most,  if  not  all,  of  the  commonly 
used  psychiatric  categories — schizoid,  depressive,  psychopathic,  psy- 
chosomatic— are  symptom-oriented.  They  are  based  on  certain  ex- 
ternal signs  of  unsuccessful  adaption.  In  practice,  an  individual  is 
diagnosed  as  neurotic  if  he  manifests  the  so-called  psychiatric  symp- 
toms which  are  restricted  to  a  certain  range  of  social  inefficiencies. 
Most  patients  come  to  the  psychiatric  clinic  not  expressing  dissatis- 
faction with  their  character,  but  requesting  relief  from  symptoms. 
The  attention  of  the  patient  and  most  preanalytic  therapists  is 
naturally  directed  to  the  painful,  and  often  terrifying  external  manifes- 
tations of  psychiatric  distress.  This  symptom  orientation  supplies 
another  reason  why  psychiatry  and  the  personality  theories  it  has 
produced  have  taken  on  the  negativistic,  neurosis-bound  cast  which 
we  have  called  the  pathology  error. 

The  attempt  to  develop  personality  theories  in  the  atmosphere  of 
the  consulting  room  and  clinic  has  resulted  in  still  another  interesting 
limitation.  The  second  half  of  this  compound  fallacy  is  caused  by  the 
fact  that  (until  the  last  decade),  of  all  neurotic  character  types,  only 
about  one  half  came  in  any  frequency  to  seek  psychotherapeutic  help. 
We  can  suspect  that  about  50  per  cent  of  individuals  with  marked 
character  distortions  (i.e.,  one  half  of  the  diagnostic  continuum)  did 
not  show  up  in  large  numbers  in  the  nineteenth  century  psychiatric 
office  because  the  very  essence  of  their  imbalance  tended  to  push  them 
away  from  dependence,  self-revelation,  and  conforming  cooperation. 

The  diagnostic  chapters  of  this  book  will  consider  this  interesting 
phenomenon  in  some  detail.   It  is  pertinent  to  the  argument  here  to 


26  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

point  out  that  a  large  percentage  of  the  maladjusted  population  has 
traditionally  received  little  psychiatric  attention.  They  were  not 
studied  because  they  did  not  come  for  psychiatric  help.  They  did  not 
seek  therapeutic  assistance  because  the  core  of  their  anxiety-reducing 
operations  was  a  compulsive  maintenance  of  povi^er,  independence, 
competitiveness,  or  defiance — interpersonal  techniques  which  pre- 
clude, under  ordinary  circumstances,  the  role  of  a  psychiatric  patient. 

Working  Principle  II:  Adjustive-Maladjustive 
Personality  Variables 

Thus,  our  personality  theories  have  not  only  been  lopsided  in  the 
direction  of  maladjusted  rather  than  normal  subjects,  but  also  limited 
by  overemphasis  on  a  narrow  fragment  of  the  over-all  neurotic  popu- 
lation. We  can  now  present  the  second  principle  upon  which  the 
interpersonal  system  is  based. 

Second  working  principle:  The  variables  of  a  personality  system 
should  be  designed  to  measure — on  the  same  continuum — the  normal, 
adjustive  aspects  of  behavior  as  ivell  as  abnormal  or  pathological 
extremes. 

In  validating  a  system  of  personality,  the  procedures  of  data  collec- 
tion should  include  samples  of  both  adjusted  and  maladjusted  subjects. 
Among  the  maladjusted  there  should  be  proportionate  empirical 
attention  to  those  subjects  whose  anxiety  is  lessened  by  rushing- 
into-a-psychiatric-clinic  as  well  as  those  whose  anxiety  is  dimin- 
ished by  a  rushing-away-from-the-interpersonal-implications-of-the- 
psychotherapeutic-situation. 

By  basing  their  conceptions  on  the  human  character  structure, 
rather  than  on  a  fractional  segment  of  symptoms,  Erikson,  Horney, 
Fromm,  and  Sullivan  have  doubled  the  range  of  personality  types.  We 
learn  that  many  apparently  successful  and  socially  approved  behavior 
extremes — the  driving  competitor,  the  overambitious  leader,  the  over- 
popular  hero — can  be  based  on  imbalanced  and  neurotic  character 
structures.  It  is  easy  to  add  the  corollary  that  many  phenomena  clas- 
sically considered  deeply  pathological — mild  autistic  withdrawals, 
moderate  unconventionality,  moderate  depressed  obsessiveness — are 
not  severe  imbalances  but  constructive,  healthy,  and  perfectly  ac- 
ceptable methods  of  warding  off  anxiety. 

Effect  of  Cultural  Values  on  Theories  of  Normality 

Fromm  speaks  in  this  connection  of  the  difference  between  per- 
sonal and  social  maladjustment.  Social  efficiency  manifested  by  public 
esteem,  high  income,  and  feverish  productivity  may  give  the  appear- 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  27 

ance  of  healthy  adjustment  at  the  expense  of  disequilibrium  and  in- 
ternal distortion.  Social  inefficiency,  defined  in  terms  of  low  income, 
nonconformity,  modest  station,  social  introversion,  and  relaxed  am- 
bition does  not  always  indicate  unhappiness  or  psychic  disturbance. 
Poets  have  known  this  for  some  centuries. 

The  basic  values  of  the  American  middle  class,  which  insidiously 
permeate  all  of  its  members,  exert  their  influence  on  contemporary 
psychiatric  theories.  It  is  very  easy  to  identify  normality  with  con- 
ventionality or  optimistic,  active,  responsible  independence;  and 
neurosis  with  nonconformity  or  pessimistic,  inactive  sensitivity. 

The  definition  of  adjustment  is  thus  complicated  by  the  inevitable 
pressure  of  value  systems:  Is  it  more  "normal"  to  express  constructive, 
conjunctive,  conventional  affiUative  feelings?  Is  it  more  "abnormal" 
to  manifest  distrustful,  hostile,  rebellious  behavior? 

The  personality  theorist  need  not  base  his  definition  on  cultural 
values,  but  it  is  certainly  necessary  to  take  into  account  the  social  and 
ethical  esteem  which  attaches  to  certain  popular  security  operations. 

There  are  two  issues  which  must  be  faced — a  quantitative  and  a 
qualitative  consideration  of  adjustment. 

Quantitative  Definition  of  Adjustment 

This  book  is  presenting  a  system  for  diagnosing  personality  which 
strives  to  be  objective  and  operational.  This  commits  us  to  a  quan- 
titative definition  of  maladjustment.  We  set  up  continua  for  measur- 
ing or  classifying  interpersonal  behavior  in  terms  of  several  indices. 
Normality-abnormality  is  defined  in  terms  of  these  indices. 

The  first  of  these  quantitative  scales  concerns  consistent  modera- 
tion versus  intensity  at  any  one  level  of  behavior.  The  former  is  con- 
sidered adjustive,  the  second  maladjustive. 

The  second  categorization  concerns  flexibility  versus  rigidity  at 
any  one  level  of  behavior.  The  former  is  considered  adjustive,  the 
latter  maladjustive. 

A  third  quantitative  index  of  normality  involves  the  stability  or 
oscillation  among  different  levels  of  personality.  Extreme  conflict 
(oscillation)  among  levels  is  viewed  as  maladjustive.  So  is  extreme 
interlevel  rigidity,  i.e.,  the  same  interpersonal  operations  repeated  at 
all  levels.  Stable  or  balanced  interlevel  patterns  are  seen  as  adjustive. 

A  fourth  (and  less  clear-cut)  definition  of  normality  involves 
measurements  of  accuracy  and  appropriateness.  If  behavior  is  in- 
appropriate, if  perceptions  are  inaccurate,  then  maladjustment  is 
indicated. 

The  methodology  and  specific  apphcation  of  these  quantitative  in- 
dices will  be  described  in  later  sections  of  this  book. 


2  8  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

Qualitative  Definition  of  Adjustment 

A  second  approach  to  the  definition  of  adjustment  and  maladjust- 
ment involves  a  qualitative  assessment  of  behavior.  Here  we  do  not 
ask  "how  much?"  or  "how  rigid?"  or  "how  accurate?"  but  concen- 
trate on  luhat  kind  of  interpersonal  behavior. 

The  qualitative  definition  of  normality  is  inextricably  rooted  in 
value  judgments  and  does  not  appear  to  be  useful  in  developing  an 
objective  diagnostic  system.  The  quantitative  concept  of  adjustment 
is  based  on  the  notion  of  personal  adjustment.  How  balanced,  ac- 
curate, adaptable  are  the  security  operations?  How  successful  are  they 
in  warding  off  anxiety?  The  qualitative  concept  is  based  on  social 
adjustment — conformity  to  cultural  stereotypes  as  to  what  is  normal. 

Let  us  grant  that  no  human  being  is  perfectly  balanced,  and  that 
everyone  has  developed  modes  of  dealing  with  anxiety  which  em- 
phasize certain  interpersonal  behaviors  and  minimize  others.  The 
qualitative  question  then  becomes:  Are  there  socially  preferred  kinds 
of  security  operations?  Are  there  certain  modes  of  response  which 
are  intrinsically  better  than  others? 

Is  conventionality  or  loving  trust,  for  example,  intrinsically  more 
adjusted  than  bitter  rebellion? 

There  is  no  answer  to  these  questions.  This  is  a  cultural,  ethical 
issue.  The  neutral  position  of  the  scientist  (which  of  course  is  an  ideal 
and  never  an  actuality)  can  be  preserved  by  accepting  explicitly 
quantitative  definitions  of  adjustment  and  avoiding  (as  far  as  it  is  pos- 
sible) the  qualitative. 

By  way  of  illustration,  let  us  consider  two  patients,  both  of  whom 
have  intense  underlying  feelings  of  despair  and  a  long  history  of 
deprivation  and  derogation.  One  patient  reacts  to  these  inner  feelings 
and  experiences  by  means  of  a  rigid  conventionality  and  conformity 
to  duty.  The  second  patient  reacts  to  the  same  inner  feelings  and  the 
same  unhappy  history  by  means  of  a  rigid  rebellion  and  bitter  rejec- 
tion of  conventional  behavior. 

Assuming  the  rigidity  and  intensity  of  the  two  security  operations 
to  be  equal,  is  one  more  adjustive  than  the  other?  A  quantitative 
definition  would  hold  that  there  is  no  difference. 

A  qualitative  definition  might  tend  to  consider  one  more  normal 
than  the  other.  Certainly,  most  cultural,  ethical  values  would  prefer 
the  former  conforming,  cooperative  operations  and  disapprove  of  the 
latter.  But  from  the  standpoint  of  the  individual  and  his  quest  for  se- 
curity it  will  be  seen  that  both  may  achieve  the  same  amount  of  self- 
esteem  and  suffer  from  the  same  amount  of  conflict.  They  may  be 
equally  successful  in  warding  off  anxiety. 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  29 

Large  and  diverse  samples  of  subjects  studied  by  means  of  dis- 
ciplined, logical  variable  systems  offer  the  best  protection  against  one- 
sided success-oriented  personality  theories.  The  invaluable  assistance 
of  formal  classification  and  notational  structures  in  systematizing  the 
data  of  human  nature  is  one  of  the  basic  maxims  of  this  book.  The 
following  chapter  is  devoted  to  this  topic.  The  following  example  will 
serve  to  illustrate  its  usefulness  in  the  context  of  the  present  discussion 
of  social  versus  personal  adjustment. 

Illustration  of  the  Impact  of  Cultural  Values 
on  Conceptions  of  Moral  Character 

In  the  process  of  developing  a  systematic  list  of  interpersonal 
variables  it  is  obvious  that  hostile  and  affectionate  behaviors  are  among 
the  commonly  employed  means  of  dealing  with  others.  When  we 
apply  the  principles  of  the  normality-abnormality  continuum,  it  fol- 
lows logically  that  we  must  have  linguistic  terms  for  describing  inter- 
mediate points  along  the  continuum  between  these  two  interpersonal 
motives.  This  is  to  say,  we  must  measure  the  moderate-adaptive  and 
the  intense  pathological  extremes  of  each  morive.  Thus,  in  devising 
rating  scales,  diagnostic  terms,  test  check  lists  and  the  like,  it  is  formally 
required  that  we  have  signs  or  terms  to  reflect  the  adjustively  hostile, 
the  adjustively  affectionate,  the  maladaptively  hostile,  and  the  mal- 
adaptively  affectionate. 

When  the  Kaiser  Foundation  psychology  research  project  began 
to  develop  a  system  of  interpersonal  variables,  a  puzzling  linguistic 
situation  was  uncovered.  It  became  clear  that  the  English  language — 
whether  that  of  the  psychiatrist  or  that  of  the  general  public — has  a 
marked  imbalance  in  the  number  of  terms  which  describe  different 
interpersonal  themes.  There  was  no  trouble  in  obtaining  long  columns 
of  words  describing  the  positive,  socially  adaptive  expressions  of 
friendliness,  amiability,  love,  agreeability,  etc.  Nor  was  there  difficulty 
in  listing  maladjustive,  pathologically  toned  denotations  of  extreme 
hostility,  hatred,  opposition,  rage,  etc.  It  was,  however,  a  tedious  task 
to  get  three  or  four  commonly  used  words  for  the  concept  of  adjustive, 
socially  approved  hostility.  Considerable  dictionary,  thesaurus,  and 
literary  research  uncovered  a  few  such  words — frank,  blunt,  critical — 
but  it  appears  that  the  English  language,  and  the  implicit  folk  con- 
ceptions of  human  nature  that  underly  it,  pay  little  attention  to  the 
theme  of  appropriate  expression  of  disaffiliative  interpersonal  behavior. 

Interpersonal  check  lists  were  given  to  large  samples  of  diverse  sub- 
jects in  order  to  obtain  a  balanced  variable  system  and  to  determine  the 
expected  frequency  of  social  motivations  attributed  to  self.  The  logic 
of  the  personality  system  and  statistical  simplicity  demanded  a  balance 


30  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

between  hostile  and  friendly  terms,  but  the  one-sidedness  of  the  inter- 
personal terminology  and  conceptualization  of  Anglo-American  cul- 
ture made  it  necessary  to  employ  such  clumsy  terms  as  righteous 
anger,  not  afraid  to  be  critical,  and  the  like,  in  order  to  express  the 
theme  of  adaptive,  appropriate  hostility. 

When  we  seek  to  find  terms  which  express  extreme,  rigid  mal- 
adjustive  affectionate  behavior,  the  problem  becomes  insoluble. 
There  are  no  such  simple  words  in  the  language.  According  to  our 
linguistic  forefathers,  the  human  being  cannot  be  too  loving.  The 
notion  that  one  can  be  neurotically  or  compulsively  affiliative  is 
literally  unthought  of. 

In  this  instance,  the  logical  principles  of  the  normality-abnormality 
continuum  of  interpersonal  behavior  and  the  discipline  of  a  formal 
notational  system  lead  to  some  interesting  semantic,  anthropological 
speculations  and  a  further  illustration  of  the  one-sided  clinical  error. 

What  Is  Adjustment? 

In  pointing  out  the  limitations  of  classical  psychiatry,  and  in  advo- 
cating expanded  symmetrical,  logical  principles  for  dealing  with  the 
normality-abnormaUty  continuum,  we  have  left  untouched  two  vital 
questions:  What  is  normality?  What  is  neurosis?  These  are  crucial 
issues  because  the  theoretical  position  assumed  on  these  questions  is 
inextricably  bound  to  the  resulting  conceptions  of  personality  organi- 
zation, diagnosis,  and  therapeutic  orientation. 

Horney  presents  changing  and  developing  definitions  of  neurosis  in 
her  different  publications.  In  general,  she  appears  to  see  normality  as 
flexibility,  optimal  productivity,  as  well  as  a  relative  emancipation  from 
anxiety  and  the  conflicts  which  accompany  it.  Fromm  stresses  produc- 
tiveness, responsibility,  mature  affection,  understanding,  a  rational 
handling  of  the  authority  relationship,  and  "freedom"  from  irrational 
dependence.  Sullivan  defines  mental  health  as  accurate,  mutually  re- 
warding interpersonal  relationships.  All  of  these  authors  are  aware  of 
the  effect  of  the  culture  on  our  conception  of  normality.  They  point 
out  that  deviation  from  the  norm  must  be  viewed  in  the  context  of  the 
social  background.  When  Sullivan  ties  his  most  adequate  mode  of  ex- 
perience— the  syntaxic — to  consensual  validation  he  recognizes  cul- 
tural relativity,  and  holds  that  a  "great  deal  of  most  people's  syntaxic 
experience  is  bound  by  the  prescriptions  and  limitations  of  the 
culture  .  .  ." 

When  we  survey  these  criteria  of  normality,  two  thoughts  may 
occur.  First,  they  are  all  partially  vahd,  in  the  sense  that  they  refer  to 
aspects  of  adjustive  functioning.  Second,  none  of  them  is  complete, 
systematic,  or  too  well  organized.    Productivity,  syntaxic  function, 


ADJUSTMENT-MALADJUSTMENT  FACTORS  31 

and  achievement  of  one's  potential  are  broad  concepts,  admirable 
foundations  for  a  philosophy  of  human  nature,  but  much  too  vague 
and  general  to  be  used  as  research  and  clinical  variables. 

From  the  standpoint  of  operational  measurement,  most  definitions 
of  normality  are  either  too  specific,  and  thus  fragmentary,  or  too 
broad,  and  thus  imprecise.  This  is  because  normality  cannot  be  sys- 
tematically defined  until  a  comprehensive  system  exists  for  organizing 
the  multiplex  data  of  human  nature.  Personality  processes  operate  at 
many  levels  and  in  many  forms.  The  nature  of  the  definition  of 
neurosis  is  always  chained  to  the  nature  of  the  system  of  variables  by 
w^hich  the  theorist  classifies  human  behavior. 

We  shall  obtain  rigorous,  logical,  complex  heuristic  definitions  of 
adjustment-maladjustment  when  we  are  given  systematic  multilevel 
definitions  of  human  personality.  Until  then  the  conception  of 
neurosis  will  reflect  the  level  of  personality  to  which  the  theorist  is 
limited. 

At  this  point  in  the  discussion  it  is  appropriate  to  introduce  the 
theory  of  normality  basic  to  the  personality  system  presented  in  this 
book.  To  venture  its  definition  at  this  early  stage  of  the  exposition  is  a 
hazardous  proposition.  Since  a  detailed  description  of  personality 
organization  has  not  been  presented,  a  detailed  definition  of  normality 
is  premature.  We  shall  be  forced  to  employ  undefined  words,  refer  to 
undefined  levels  and  their  undescribed  relationships.  Fluent  expres- 
sion of  nonoperationally  defined  terms  is  the  easiest  trap  that  awaits 
the  personality  theorist.  We  shall,  with  these  reservations,  present  a 
verbal  description  of  normality,  at  the  same  time  referring  the  reader 
ahead  to  the  systematic  and  operationally  defined  categories  which  are 
to  follow  in  Chapter  12. 

Adjustment  in  terms  of  the  over-all  personality  organization  con- 
sists in  flexible,  balanced,  appropriate,  accurate  interpersonal  behavior. 
In  terms  of  the  subdivisions  of  personality — the  levels  of  public  inter- 
action, perception,  and  private  symbolism — it  consists  of  appropriate, 
accurate,  and  balanced  interpersonal  behavior  respectively.  When  we 
re-examine  this  definition  we  shall  see  that  each  term  has  a  rigorous 
quantitative  meaning — referring  to  specific,  operationally  defined 
processes.  In  the  broad  scope,  we  call  normality  an  equilibrium  of  all 
the  levels  of  personality  such  that  the  necessary  mild  character  distor- 
tions at  some  levels  are  moderately  counterbalanced  at  other  levels.  A 
different  subdefinition  exists  for  each  different  level  of  personality.  At 
the  level  of  perception  of  self  or  others,  accuracy  or  syntaxic  agree- 
ment with  consensual  perception  is  a  partial  index  of  adjustment.  At 
the  level  of  overt  interaction,  the  proportion  of  flexible  interactions 
appropriate  to  the  interpersonal  stimulus  becomes  the  index  of  adjust- 


32  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

ment.  At  the  level  of  indirect,  fantasy  expression,  the  breadth  of 
symbolic  themes  and  their  balance  and  relationship  to  the  other  levels 
provides  the  ratio  of  adjustment/ 

The  verbal  definition  of  adjustment  presented  above  rests  upon  one 
basic  (philosophic)  assumption:  survival  anxiety  as  the  motivating 
force  of  interpersonal  behavior.  This  premise  shapes  the  resulting 
theory  of  normality.  It  also  focuses  on  certain  types  of  variables 
(interpersonal),  and  requires  certain  formal  multilevel  systems  for 
relating  these  variables.  The  conception  of  adjustment-maladjust- 
ment presented  in  this  section,  therefore,  does  not  stand  as  an  isolated 
verbal  entity.  This  will  become  clearer  as  we  examine,  in  later  chap- 
ters, the  specific  and,  in  the  following  chapter,  the  general  principles 
of  the  system  on  which  it  is  based. 

'  In  Chapter  12  operational  methods  for  classifying  and  diagnosing  behavior  will  be 
presented.  This  conception  of  adjustment  is  based  on  the  notions  of  moderation,  bal- 
ance, and  flexibility.  In  developing  objective  criteria  for  measuring  these  qualities  we 
have  found  ourselves  borrowing  from  certain  historical  antecedents  and  rejecting 
others.  Moderation  and  the  avoidance  of  extremes  is,  of  course,  the  definition  of 
adjustment  sponsored  by  Aristotle.  Flexibility  and  the  avoidance  of  narrow,  rigid 
forms  of  adjustment  is  the  Renaissance  ideal.  The  Christian  conception  of  values 
views  normality  as  a  victor  over  man's  intrinsic  evil  nature.  This  notion  is  reflected 
in  the  psychiatric  theories  of  adjustment  developed  in  the  nineteentli  century.  It  is  a 
curious  irony  that  empirical  approaches  to  the  definition  of  normality  find  their 
intellectual  heritage  in  the  Greek  and  Renaissance  philosophies  which  are  more  distant 
in  many  other  respects  from  the  ethos  of  t%ventieth-century  culture. 


References 

1.  Fromm,  E.   Escape  from  freedom.   New  York:  Rinehart,  1951. 

2.  Masserman,   J.   H.    Principles   of  dynamic   psychiatry.    Philadelphia:    Saunders, 
1946. 

3.  MuLLAHY,  p.   The  theories  of  Harry  Stack  Sullivan.  In  P.  Mullahy   (ed.).  The 
Contributions  of  Harry  Stack  Sullivan.  New  York:  Hermitage  House,  1952. 

4.  Proust,  M.  Swann's  way.  Translated  by  C-  K.  Scott-MoncriefT.  London:  Chatto, 
1922.  Vol.  1. 

5.  RuEscH,  J.,  and  G.  Bateson.   Communication.   New  York:  Norton,  1951. 


Systematizing  the  Complexity 
of  Personality 


That  segment  of  personality  which  we  have  selected  to  systematize 
centers  on  adjustive  and  maladjustive  interpersonal  behavior.  Even 
when  we  narrow  our  field  to  the  social  dimension  of  personality,  the 
systematic  task  remaining  is  terribly  complicated.  The  diversity  of 
interpersonal  behavior  covers  a  wide  range.  It  includes  all  the  things  a 
subject  does  to  others  at  all  levels  of  personality — overtly,  symboli- 
cally, and  in  private  perceptions.  When  we  add  the  parallel  behaviors 
of  others  who  do  things  to  the  subject  we  obtain  a  network  of  events 
that  probably  equals  in  complexity  the  data  of  the  physical  sciences. 
When  we  consider  further  the  effects  of  culture,  sex  difference,  and 
the  peculiarly  self-deceptive  nature  of  emotional  data,  the  enormity  of 
the  scientific  task  becomes  clear. 

In  undertaking  this  complex  mission,  personality  psychology  can, 
fortunately,  count  on  some  conceptual  assistance — new  developments 
in  the  philosophy  of  science.  In  recent  years  considerable  progress 
has  been  made  by  a  group  of  logicians  and  positivist  philosophers 
which  is  directly  apphcable  to  the  field  of  personality.  The  study  of 
human  nature  can  find  guide  posts  in  the  general  principles  which 
guide  the  physical  sciences. 

The  Basic  Conceptual  Unit  of  Personality 

We  shall  begin  by  considering  a  preliminary  question.  When  we 
study  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  an  individual,  what  is  the  basic 
datum  on  which  we  make  our  judgments?  The  first  answer  to  this 
question  might  be  that  we  employ  a  variety  of  behavioral  cues:  projec- 
tive personality  tests,  tales  of  woe  from  the  interview,  the  angry  tones 
of  voice,  dream  texts,  and  the  hke.  These  are,  it  is  true,  the  events,  but 

33 


34  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

they  are  not  the  basic  data  for  the  study  of  personality.  How  can  we 
measure  these  written,  oral,  and  physical  expressions  in  such  a  way  as 
to  provide  comparative  conceptual  material?  It  is  possible,  but  rarely 
feasible,  to  capture  these  events  by  sound  and  movie  equipment.  Even 
then  we  must  decide  what  to  do  with  these  unwieldy  materials  when 
we  get  them. 

For  many  years  researchers  have  been  working  within  one  or  an- 
other of  these  areas  of  raw  personality  data,  painfully  building  up  com- 
plex devices  for  categorizing  the  different  surface  types  of  expression. 
Hundreds  of  systems  for  dealing  with  personality  tests  have  been  pub- 
lished. We  have  learned,  to  our  horror,  that  it  is  possible  to  devise 
measurement  scales  for  each  facet  of  personahty  expression.  Thus,  it 
is  possible  to  have  an  elaborate  continuum  for  rating  each  type  of  test, 
another  for  measuring  the  amount  of  sadness  or  depression  expressed 
by  the  subject,  another  for  classifying  the  nuances  of  tone  of  voice. 
None  of  these  scales  need  any  relationship  to  each  other,  and  they 
leave  unsolved  the  great  paradox  that  personality  must  be  considered 
as  somewhat  unified  yet  is  expressed  in  a  variety  of  ways. 

Actually,  a  distressing  amount  of  creative  energy  has  gone  into 
molecular,  stimulus-bound  research  of  this  sort.  One  method  of  clas- 
sifying the  responses  to  one  test,  the  Rorschach  ink  blots,  involves  over 
sixty  elaborate  and  tricky  rating  procedures.  These  variables  have 
direct  reference  only  to  the  ink  blots  themselves,  and  by  circuitous 
and  generally  unvalidated  intuition  refer  to  a  few  aspects  of  general 
behavior.  This  is  a  single  example  of  the  unfortunate  and  common 
practice  of  chasing  one  aspect  of  raw  personality  data  down  a  tortuous 
side  alley. 

We  have  several  score  of  personality  tests,  each  of  which  employs 
tedious  methods  for  summarizing  an  extremely  artificial  and  narrow 
range  of  expressive  behavior.  Most  of  these  tests  force  the  develop- 
ment of  miniature  personality  theories  which  work  for  the  tiny  seg- 
ment of  behavior  that  they  tap.  A  test  which  uses  sand  and  water  as 
part  of  the  stimulus  items  thus  employs  a  theory  which  gives  sand  and 
water  a  prominent  role  in  personality  development. 

The  solution  we  have  employed  to  deal  with  this  unsatisfactory 
situation  is  to  define  as  the  basic  data  of  personahty,  not  the  expressive 
events,  but  the  communications  by  the  subject  or  by  others  about  his 
interpersonal  activity.  The  basic  units  of  personality  come  from  the 
protocol  language  by  which  the  subject's  interpersonal  behavior  is 
described. 

When  the  subject  smiles  we  attend  to  it,  but  the  smile  is  not  the 
datum  which  directly  concerns  us.  Someone  who  is  present  in  the 
situation,  or  observes  it  in  cinematic  form,  has  to  make  a  protocol 


THE  COMPLEXITY  OF  PERSONALITY  35 

statement  about  this  movement  of  facial  muscles  before  it  becomes  a 
datum  of  personality.  We  study  not  the  actual  behavior,  but  the 
language  about  it  (including  the  subject's  language  about  it). 

This  may  sound,  at  first  impression,  like  a  restricting  definition.  But 
when  we  remember  that  we  can  obtain  many  descriptions  of  the  same 
momentary  event,  it  actually  provides  a  systematic  way  of  multiplying 
our  knowledge.  The  smile,  for  example,  might  elicit  many  data  sen- 
tences. The  subject  himself  might  describe  his  motive  purpose  at  the 
moment  as  friendliness.  The  consensual  report  of  many  judges  might 
agree  in  attributing  friendly  purpose  to  the  smile.  A  suspicious  relative, 
however,  might  judge  it  as  smug  or  patronizing.  A  dependent  relative 
might  attribute  tender  sympathy.  Thus,  this  facial  gesture  produces 
many  protocol  statements  which  provide  interpersonal  information 
about  the  subject's  description  of  self  and  his  social  stimulus  value  to 
others. 

The  basic  data  of  personality  studied  by  the  interpersonal  system 
are  the  verbal  protocol  statements  about  interpersonal  behavior, 
i.e.,  the  language  in  which  the  subject  or  others  describe  his  inter- 
personal interactions,  perceptions,  and  symbols.  The  diverse  molecular 
responses — tears,  bodily  movements,  test  reactions — are  the  raw  ma- 
terials. From  them  we  obtain  the  building  blocks  for  the  scientific 
study  of  personality.  These  are  units  of  classification — terms  such  as 
depressed,  angry,  confident. 

The  Structure  of  Scientific  Language 

In  the  methodological  aspects  of  the  science,  we  use  a  wide  variety 
of  empirical  techniques  to  obtam  the  raw  data  of  personality.  We 
utilize  these  direct  observations  by  converting  them  into  systematic 
protocol  language.  Scientific  study  of  personality  consists  in  a  study 
of  the  systematic  language  by  which  we  describe  the  many  facets  of 
behavior.  These  conceptual  operations  refer  to  the  formal  aspects  of 
the  science. 

This  important  division  of  scientific  procedures  into  empirical  and 
formal  propositions  has  developed  out  of  the  scientific  philosophy  of 
the  twentieth  century.  Bertrand  Russell  and  the  Logical  Positivists 
(Wittgenstein,  Carnap,  etc.)  have  helped  to  make  the  distinction  be- 
tween the  synthetic  operational  language,  which  refers  to  measurable 
events  in  the  physical-social  world,  and  the  formal  analytic  procedures 
by  which  the  language  of  science  is  organized. 

These  two  distinct  types  of  scientific  communications  were  rede- 
fined and  a  third  pragmatic  function  added  by  C.  W.  Morris.  This 
American  philosopher  claimed  that  all  scientific  activity  can  be  studied 
as  forms  of  the  language  of  science.  The  general  science  which  studies 


35  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

the  entire  field  of  scientific  communication  he  calls  semiotic.  He 
defines  three  different  functions  of  scientific  behavior:  ( 1 )  Semantics 
studies  the  relation  of  signs  to  objects  and  thus  covers  the  empirical, 
experimental,  and  methodological  aspects  of  science.  (2)  Syntactics 
is  concerned  with  the  relation  of  signs  to  signs,  and  involves  the  formal 
procedures  of  logic,  syntax,  and  mathematics.  (3)  Pragmatics  deals 
with  the  relation  of  signs  to  the  users  of  signs.  This  branch  of  semiotic 
studies  the  functional  and  applied  meaning  of  communicative  behavior. 
Let  us  examine  Morris'  three  functions  in  more  detail. 

(1)  Every  science  has  unique  methods  and  variables  for  dealing 
with  its  specific  data.  These  variables  and  their  relationships  are  de- 
scribed in  terms  of  language.  Thus,  despite  the  great  variations  in  what 
scientists  do  with  their  various  data,  the  net  result  always  involves 
communication  or  sign  behavior. 

Certain  general  rules  hold  for  all  empirical  investigations.  Among 
these  we  include  the  need  for  unambiguous  operational  definitions  of 
terms,  and  the  need  for  public  and  repeatable  measurements,  pro- 
cedures, and  the  like.  Morris  calls  these  semantic  rules  since  they  gov- 
ern the  relationship  of  signs  to  the  empirical  events.  All  sciences  differ, 
but  all  must  conform  to  the  same  standards  of  objectivity. 

(2)  These  empirical  propositions  which  are  related  to  observable 
and  testable  facts  are  crucially  different  from  the  formal  prepositional 
structures  of  a  science.  The  latter  comprise  systems  which  regulate 
the  relationship  of  signs  or  language  units.  They  have  no  empirical 
reference.  Such  formal  devices  are  indispensable  because  they  deter- 
mine how  the  researcher  organizes  his  factual  language.  Mathematics 
and  the  logical  deductive  systems  employed  by  modem  science  do  not 
depend  upon  empirical  proof.  They  are,  in  this  sense,  complex  sets  of 
terms  which  are  inflexibly  related  to  each  other  according  to  pre- 
established,  assumed  rules.  The  arithmetical  statement  "two  times 
five  equals  ten,"  for  example,  is  a  predetermined  relationship  based  on 
our  original  definition  of  what  each  of  the  terms  means.  This  sentence 
is  therefore  empty  of  factual  meaning.  The  psychoanalytic  statement 
"the  ego  wards  off  instinctual  impulses,"  is  similarly  formal,  depend- 
ing on  the  assumed  relationship  of  ego  and  instinct.  It  has  no  empirical 
meaning. 

(3)  The  pragmatic  aspects  of  the  language  of  personality  delimit 
a  broad  and  ramified  field.  They  refer  to  the  sociology  of  our  psy- 
chological knowledge,  its  pohtics,  its  practical  application  in  diagnosis 
and  therapy.  We  have  found  it  necessary  to  narrow  the  scope  of  the 
pragmatics  of  our  system  to  the  predictive  function  in  the  psychiatric 
clinic.  We  have  selected  the  interpersonal  framework  because  it  ap- 
pears to  be  the  most  functional  in  terms  of  survival  of  the  individual 


THE  COMPLEXITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


37 


and  a  critical  prediction  of  clinical  events.  In  due  course  we  shall 
attempt  to  show  that  every  variable  and  every  diagnostic  category 
presented  in  this  book  has  been  chosen  to  predict  directly  the 
crucial  aspects  of  the  subject's  future  behavior — particularly  with  the 
future  therapist.  Thus  we  equate  the  pragmatics  of  personality  psy- 
chology with  prediction.  From  the  standpoint  of  psychiatric  opera- 
tions— the  orientation  of  this  book — nothing  is  so  important  as  to 
have  probability  knowledge  of  the  patient's  future  pattern  of  inter- 
personal behavior.  This  interpretation  of  the  pragmatics  of  person- 
ality is,  of  course,  the  narrow  sector  of  the  broad  field  outlined  by 
Morris  that  is  most  pertinent  to  a  clinical  psychology. 

With  this  threefold  classification  in  mind,  let  us  return  to  the  dis- 
tinction between  empirical  and  formal  propositions.  Since  empirical 
statements  are  related  to  and  are  limited  to  observable  events,  and  since 
formal  statements  are  not,  it  is  of  critical  importance  to  distinguish 
between  the  two  types  of  propositions.  Failure  to  do  so  leads  to  dan- 
gerous fallacies.  These  generally  involve  tautological  formal  state- 
ments which  appear  to  be  empirical  assertions.  The  psychoanalytic 
phrase  just  quoted,  for  example,  refers  only  to  Freud's  logical  struc- 
ture of  personality.  It  refers  to  the  relationship  between  the  language 
forms  "ego"  and  "id"  employed  by  Freud.  The  psychoanalytic  lin- 
guistic system,  which  is  the  most  ambitious  yet  developed  in  the  field 
of  personality,  has  restricted  empirical  reference.  Those  who  employ 
Freud's  verbal  conventions  often  imply  that  they  are  making  factual 
statements  rather  than  logical  tautologies.  Cripphng  confusions  and 
meaningless  communications  will  inevitably  result  if  empirical  and 
formal  statements  and  pragmatic  operations  are  not  kept  clearly 
distinct. 

If  they  are  kept  distinct  several  benefits  accrue.  The  most  im- 
portant of  these  is  the  general  ordering  of  scientific  activity.  From  the 
chaotic  complexity  of  personality  data  emerge  three  broad  and  dis- 
tinct sets  of  operations — the  empirical-methodological,  the  formal- 
logical,  and  the  practical  applications.  Personality  study  currently 
faces  these  three  challenging  tasks:  to  measure  objectively  and  mean- 
ingfully, to  relate  the  obtained  variables  systematically  and  logically, 
and  to  apply  the  resulting  knowledge  with  known  predictive  accuracy. 
We  shall  accomplish  these  objectives  most  efficiently  by  working 
within  the  principles  of  contemporary  unified  science.  The  rules  for 
empirical  methods  (reviewed  in  Chapter  4)  will  guide  our  approaches 
to  the  raw  datum,  and  its  conversion  into  rehable  language  units.  The 
formal  principles  will  assist  us  in  organizing  our  linguistic  units.  The 
goal  of  pragmatic  applicability  will  encourage  us  to  relate  our  sys- 
tematic knowledge  to  external  events  and  to  functional  issues.   Seen 


,8  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

in  this  light,  personality  psychology  becomes  part  of  a  unified  general 
science. 

The  purpose  and  outline  of  this  book  can  now  be  restated  in  terms 
of  these  three  categories.  The  remainder  of  this  chapter  presents 
some  basic  principles,  some  of  which  deal  with  a  Logic  of  Personality. 
The  two  subsequent  chapters  survey  the  empirical  and  functional 
aspects  of  the  field  of  personality.  Chapter  6  and  the  two  subsequent 
sections  (Part  II  and  Part  III)  return  to  the  same  issues,  presenting 
objective  methods  for  measuring  interpersonal  variables  and  formal 
notational  systems  for  relating  them.  In  parts  IV  and  V  the  prag- 
matic themes  assume  priority  as  we  apply  the  conceptual  system  to 
problems  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  in  and  out  of  the  psychiatric 
clinic. 

The  Selection  of  Personality  Variables 

We  began  by  noting  the  complexity  of  personality.  From  the 
philosophy  of  science  we  obtained  three  categories  of  scientific  dis- 
course which  help  bring  preliminary  order  to  this  diversity.  This 
chapter  goes  on  to  present  five  working  principles,  which  further 
assist  in  clarifying  and  systematizing  the  chaotic,  fluid  intricacy  of 
human  behavior. 

The  first  issue  concerns  the  variables,  elements,  or  conceptual  units 
to  be  employed  in  dealing  with  the  enormously  diverse  range  of 
protocol  sentences  which  describe  interpersonal  behavior.  Every 
personality  theorist  has  faced  the  formal  questions  of  how  many  ele- 
ments or  variables  of  personality  are  to  be  employed  and  how  they 
are  related.  The  first  impression  one  might  receive  from  many  pre- 
vious theorists  is  that  personality  structure  is  very  uncomplicated. 
Scores  of  dichotomous  variables  have  been  offered  as  the  basic  dimen- 
sion of  human  behavior — schizothymic  versus  cyclothymic,  intro- 
verted versus  extroverted,  etc.  As  many  three-way  classifications  have 
been  popularized — lean,  fat,  muscular;  intropunitive,  extropunitive, 
impunitive;  and  the  like.  Most  of  these  narrow  conceptual  solutions 
have  quickly  collapsed  when  asked  to  carry  the  heavy  load  of  human 
variety.  A  broad  collection  of  variables  is  a  necessary  answer  to  the 
question  of  "how  many?" 

Another,  more  elaborate  but  ineffective,  solution  to  the  problem 
of  basic  elements  is  to  employ  one  extremely  broad,  vague  variable 
such  as  libidinal  force  or  drive-towards-groivth.  Motive  concepts  of 
this  sort  allow  plenty  of  room  for  diversity  but  give  no  specific 
assistance  to  the  empirical  worker. 

A  broad  set  of  simple  and  specific  elements  (that  we  have  here  held 
to  be  necessary)  leads  to  another  formal  requirement.    Several  such 


THE  COMPLEXITY  OF  PERSONALITY  39 

systems  of  variables  have  been  developed  by  personality  theorists. 
Many  of  the  variables  in  these  systems  have  tended  to  overiap  each 
other,  to  overweight  certain  interpersonal  behaviors,  and  to  miss 
others.  They  have  not  been  related  to  each  other  in  a  systematic 
order  (i.e.,  on  a  continuum  or  scale).  Henry  Murray  published  (I), 
in  1938,  an  extensive  list  of  human  "needs"  which  has  merited  the 
considerable  usage  it  has  received.  In  a  later  publication,  Murray  has 
criticized  his  own  eclectic  collection  of  motive  variables  by  proposing 
that  social  scientists  "devote  themselves  more  resolutely  than  they 
have  so  far,  to  the  building  of  a  comprehensive  system  of  concepts 
which  are  defined  not  only  operationally  but  in  relation  to  each 
other."  (2,  p.  200)  This  demanding  proposal,  which  we  herewith 
include  in  our  list  of  working  principles,  means  that  all  variables 
should  be  related  to  each  other  along  some  kind  of  continuum.  It 
means  that  each  element  should  be  located  in  fixed  relationship  to  all 
others. 

Collecting  the  strands  we  have  been  weaving  so  far  in  this  book — 
interpersonal  orientation,  adjustment-maladjustment  continuum,  sim- 
plicity, specificity,  systematic  relatedness — we  are  ready  to  state  an- 
other working  principle  which  guides  our  approach  to  human 
personality. 

Third  working  principle:  Measurement  of  interpersonal  behavior 
requires  a  broad  collection  of  simple,  specific  variables  which  are  sys- 
tematically related  to  each  other,  and  which  are  applicable  to  the 
study  of  adjustive  or  maladjustive  responses. 

The  Logic  of  Interaction 

Another  formal  issue  must  now  be  met.  Interpersonal  behavior  has 
been  defined  as  the  basic  area  of  personahty.  It  is  in  the  essence  of 
interpersonal  phenomena  that  they  never  exist  in  isolation,  but  always 
in  interaction  with  real  or  imagined  others.  We  must  conceive  the 
interpersonal  activity  of  the  subject  as  he  sees  it,  expresses  it,  and 
symbolizes  it.  We  must,  in  addition,  include  his  perceptions  and  sym- 
bolic views  of  others,  as  well  as  the  responses  which  he  pulls  or  obtains 
from  others.  An  interaction  psychology  which  deals  with  the  issues 
of  what-people-do-to-each-other  runs  headlong  into  another  nest  of 
classic  philosophic  entanglements — the  subject-object  dichotomy. 
Here  we  need  another  principle  to  clarify  important  issues. 

Fourth  working  principle:  The  interpersonal  theory  of  per- 
sonality logically  requires  that,  for  each  variable  or  variable  system  by 
which  we  measure  the  subject's  behavior  {at  all  levels  of  personality), 
we  must  include  an  equivalent  set  for  measuring  the  behavior  of  each 
specified  ''other''  with  whom  the  subject  interacts. 


40  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

In  interpersonal  psychology  the  simplest  proposition  is  a  two-way 
proposition.  The  subject  is  always  in  observed,  perceived,  or  imagined 
interaction  with  crucial  "others."  These  "others"  may  or  may  not  be 
real  persons.  Considerations  of  methodological  economy  always  limit 
the  number  and  extent  of  the  interactions  that  we  can  study.  There- 
fore, some  "others"  never  get  measured  or  placed  on  the  summary 
charts. 

The  Multilevel  Nature  of  Personality 

We  are  engaged  in  this  chapter  in  stating  some  working  principles 
on  which  we  shall  base  an  adequate  codification  system  for  personality. 
The  task  of  organizing  personality  data  into  logical  categories  reaches 
its  climax  when  we  face  the  problem  of  levels. 

Recognition  of  the  multidimensional  aspect  of  human  nature  is  a 
landmark  in  the  development  of  personality  theory.  Freud's  demon- 
stration of  the  importance  of  unconscious  motivation  was  an  epochal 
intellectual  achievement.  The  single-minded  view  of  man  as  a  rational 
being  was  supplanted  by  a  binocular  or  multiocular  vision  of  human 
character.  It  has  revolutionized  our  concepts  of  personality.  It  has 
demonstrated  that  human  behavior  is  not  a  unified  single  process;  it 
is  not  just  what  it  appears  on  the  surface,  nor  what  it  is  consciously 
assumed  by  the  actor  to  be.  It  is  rather  a  shifting,  conflicted,  multi- 
faceted  complex  of  motives,  overt  and  covert. 

The  essence  of  modem  personality  psychology  is  its  multidimen- 
sional character. 

Commonsense  notions  about  human  nature  tend  to  be  unilevel. 
People  tend  to  think  that  what  they  consciously  believe  and  say  about 
themselves  is  the  entirety  of  their  personality.  They  are  often  quite 
unaware  of  intense  and  pressing  emotions  which  dominate  and  direct 
their  behavior. 

Experimental  and  academic  psychology  were  untU  recently  com- 
pletely unilevel.  The  notion  that  what  a  subject  reports  is  based  on 
assumptions  and  motives  which  are  not  publicly  stated  came  as  a 
great  surprise  to  the  Behaviorists. 

Most  of  the  current  research  in  the  field  of  personality  is  still  dis- 
tressingly unilevel  in  its  conception  and  research  design.  The  standard 
instruments  of  personality  research,  the  rating  scale,  the  check  lists  and 
the  Q-sort,  can  be  rendered  quite  ambiguous  by  the  introduction  of 
multilevel  logic.  A  typical  research  technique  is  to  present  a  psy- 
chological judge  with  a  test  protocol — let  us  say  an  MMPI  profile  or  a 
Rorschach  record — and  to  ask  him  to  rate  the  patient  on  a  list  of 
variables,  or  to  sort  a  list  of  descriptive  phrases  about  the  patient. 
Multilevel  logic  requires  that  this  task  be  rejected  as  meaningless.  The 


THE  COMPLEXITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


41 


questions  are  immediately  raised:  Should  I  rate  how  I  predict  he  will 
behave,  or  how  he  will  consciously  see  himself  to  be,  or  what  I  predict 
his  underlying  motives  are?  The  simple,  old-fashioned  procedure  of 
rating  the  subject  thus  breaks  down  into  three  or  four  rating  ap- 
proaches, each  of  which  may  differ  dramatically  from  the  others  at 
different  levels. 

Many  generalizations  about  results  in  personality  research  are 
similarly  crippled  by  a  unilevel  approach.  This  is  particularly  true  in 
the  case  of  psychiatric  and  psychosomatic  studies.  Statements  to  the 
effect  that  obese  patients  are  dependent,  neurodermatitis  patients  are 
guilty  and  ulcer  patients  are  passive,  are  quite  limited  in  meaning. 
They  seem  to  disregard  the  essential  and  basic  concept  of  modern  per- 
sonality theory — that  the  human  being  is  a  complex,  multilevel  pattern 
of  conflicting  motives  and  behaviors.  The  importance  of  a  multilevel 
approach  to  personality  can  now  be  formalized. 

Fifth  working  principle:  Any  statement  about  personality  must 
indicate  the  level  of  personality  to  which  it  refers. 

This  is  the  key  concept  upon  which  this  book  is  based.  It  will  be 
noted  in  the  clinical  and  descriptive  sections  of  this  book  that  no  refer- 
ence is  made  to  behavior  without  the  accompanying  designation  of 
the  level  from  which  it  comes.  Thus  we  say  that  ulcer  patients  are 
responsible  and  managerial — at  the  level  of  overt  public  behavior;  that 
hypertensive  patients  are  sweet  and  affiliative — in  their  conscious  self- 
description;  that  dermatosis  patients  are  masochistic — at  the  level  of 
imaginative  fantasy;  etc. 

The  prudish  (and  often  painful)  circumlocution  which  this  prin- 
ciple requires  leads  to  a  less  graceful  prose.  It  often  puzzles  and 
irritates  the  listener,  who  hopes  to  hear  more  definite  statements  about 
patients.  In  this  connection  we  recall  the  staff  meeting  in  which  a 
psychosomatic  research  was  being  reported.  An  interested  internist 
pressed  for  straightforward  answers  to  his  questions.  "Are  these 
patients  passive  and  dependent?"  The  reply  had  to  be  cumbersome: 
"They  are  not  at  all  passive  at  the  two  overt  levels;  they  are  sig- 
nificantly passive  and  dependent  at  the  level  of  preconscious  fantasy." 

Diagnostic  language  in  the  same  fashion  becomes  multiplied  in 
complexity  when  a  multilevel  approach  is  employed.  We  no  longer 
find  it  possible  to  rattle  off  a  single  diagnostic  label.  To  the  question, 
"Is  this  patient  schizoid?"  a  diagnostician  using  the  interpersonal  sys- 
tem of  personality  would  respond  in  three-layer  terminology.  A 
typical  answer  might  be:  "At  the  level  of  symptomatic  behavior  the 
patient  is  phobic;  at  the  level  of  conscious  self-description,  hysteric;  at 
the  level  of  the  preconscious,  intensely  schizoid." 


42  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

We  have  discovered  that  it  takes  considerable  patience  and  effort 
for  psychologists  to  train  themselves  to  think  in  multilevel  terms. 
The  behavioristic  background  of  academic  psychology  apparently 
makes  unilevel  conceptions  more  congenial.  Psychoanalysts,  on  the 
other  hand,  work  comfortably  and  naturally  in  a  multilevel  idiom, 
although  they  are  somewhat  uneasy  when  their  freedom  to  swoop 
from  level  to  level  is  threatened  by  the  limitations  of  operational 
definitions. 

The  Logic  of  Levels 

The  concept  of  multilevel  behavior  has  immeasurably  deepened  our 
understanding  of  human  nature.  In  addition  to  revising  most  of  our 
psychological  notions,  it  has  broadened  our  interpretations  of  artistic, 
literary,  and  historical  activity.  Along  with  these  intellectual  boons, 
however,  came  a  host  of  new  problems  and  confusions.  Much  fal- 
lacious thinking  has  based  itself  on  the  conscious-unconscious  dichot- 
omy. Formal  systems  for  clarifying  the  illogical  language  of  dynamic 
psychiatry  seem  to  be  needed.  The  next  few  decades  will  undoubtedly 
witness  the  introduction  of  many  new  systems  of  personality.  Al- 
though the  content  of  the  theories  may  vary,  it  is  hard  to  conceive  of  a 
personality  theory  (in  this  post-Freudian  era)  which  does  not  deal 
with  the  problem  of  levels.  It  seems  inevitable  that  systematic  and 
logical  rules  must  be  developed  for  dealing  with  the  multidimensional 
aspects  of  personaHty  data.  The  following  principles  seem  to  be  so 
axiomatic  as  to  hold  for  all  such  personality  theories. 

Sixth  working  principle:  The  levels  of  personality  employed  in 
any  theoretical  system  must  be  specifically  listed  and  defined.  Once 
the  logical  system  of  levels  and  relationships  among  levels  is  defined,  it 
cannot  be  changed  without  revising  all  previous  references  to  levels. 

Illogical  procedures  will  nullify  the  most  brilliant  concepts.  Good 
logic,  on  the  contrary,  is  one  of  the  most  powerful  instruments  we  can 
use  in  forging  a  theory.  The  postulates  just  suggested  for  dealing  with 
the  problems  of  levels  inevitably  force  an  increase  in  theoretical  pre- 
cision and  scope.  Listing  and  defining  levels  leads  to  improvements  in 
empirical  operations  by  clarifying  the  different  sources  of  data  con- 
tributing to  each  level  of  personality.  This  procedure  has  led  us,  for 
example,  to  the  discovery  that  different  probability  laws  hold  for  the 
different  levels.  Defining  the  formal  relationships  among  the  levels 
immediately  reveals  overlaps,  tautologies,  and  previously  undefined 
relationships  of  considerable  theoretical  promise.  The  conceptual 
issues  of  conflict,  discrepancy,  and  motivating  forces  become  sharp- 
ened.   New  conceptual  entities  become  apparent.    New  research 


THE  COMPLEXITY  OF  PERSONALITY  43 

hypotheses  develop.  Indicating  and  consistently  maintaining  the  levels 
of  the  data  allow  language  usage  to  become  more  public  and  precise. 
A  final  and  perhaps  most  important  advantage  of  notational  systems 
is  that  good  logic  breeds  better  logic.  Any  formal  system  should  re- 
veal its  own  limitations  and  restricting  assumptions.  This,  in  turn, 
helps  to  father  new  and  improved  generations  of  successors. 

Multilevel  Relatedness  of  Variables 

This  chapter  has  been  concerned  with  organizing  the  complexity 
of  behavior  into  orderly  classifications.  Four  working  principles  have 
been  presented.  They  refer  to  variable  systems  and  the  levels  of  be- 
havior at  which  the  systems  are  employed.  Before  this  discussion  is 
concluded,  one  final  principle  must  be  discussed. 

Seventh  working  principle:  The  same  variable  system  should  be 
employed  to  measure  interpersonal  behavior  at  all  levels  of  personality. 

This  means  that  we  shall  use  the  same  classificatory  elements  regard- 
less of  the  level  of  the  data.  Most  dynamic  or  multilevel  systems  of 
personality  do  not  follow  this  suggestion.  They  employ  one  classi- 
ficatory language  for  covert,  underlying  themes  and  another  language 
for  describing  overt  behavior. 

There  is  a  significant  advantage  in  using  the  same  variable  system  at 
all  levels.  It  is  possible  to  make  direct  comparisons  between  levels.  It 
is  possible  to  measure  discrepancies,  conflicts,  or  concordances  among 
levels.  These  measurable  indices  of  discrepancy,  which  we  call  indices 
of  variability  (some  of  which  are  like  the  traditional  defense  mech- 
anisms), are  useful  in  several  ways.  They  fill  out  our  clinical  picture 
of  the  personality  by  providing  quantitative  indices  of  the  amount  and 
kind  of  interlevel  conflict.  They  are  valuable  indications  of  the  inter- 
level  organization  of  personality.  They  make  possible  objective  re- 
search into  such  concepts  as  identification,  repression,  and  idealization. 

Summary 

The  themes  of  this  chapter  are  the  complexity  of  personality  and 
the  requirements  for  dealing  with  it  systematically.  The  general 
strategy  to  be  employed  should  now  be  clear.  First,  we  set  up  a  broad 
variable  system  of  interrelated  variables.  We  use  this  to  classify  the 
interpersonal  behavior  of  the  subject  and  his  world  at  several  levels 
of  personality. 

The  essence  of  this  approach  is  that  we  obtain  thousands  of  single, 
specific,  reliable  molecular  measurements.  This  makes  for  an  objective 
system.   We  get  at  the  complexity  of  personality  by  setting  up  the 


44  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

system  of  levels,  then  studying  and  comparing  a  pattern  of  hundreds 
of  scores  at  the  different  levels. 

We  do  not  employ  clinical  rating  or  intuitive  judgments;  although 
these  are  often  broad,  penetrating,  and  give  a  well-rounded  picture  of 
the  personality,  they  are  notoriously  unreliable  and  unduplicable. 
For  this  reason  we  do  not  use  professional  psychological  ratings  at 
any  point  in  the  organizing  of  data.  The  procedure  of  automatically 
sorting  thousands  of  reliable  unilevel  ratings  into  a  standardized  multi- 
level system  allows  us  to  pay  some  respect  to  the  complexity  of  per- 
sonality without  sacrifice  of  objectivity. 


References 

1.  Murray,  H.  A.   Explorations  in  personality:  A  clinical  and  experimental  study  of 
fifty  men  of  college  age.   New  York:  Oxford,  1938. 

2.  Murray,  H.  A.  Research  planning:  A  few  proposals.  In  S.  S.  Sargent  and  Marian 
W.  Smith  (eds.).  Culture  and  personality.  New  York:  Viking  Fund,  1949. 


Empirical  Principles  in 
Personality  Research 


In  the  preceding  chapter  it  was  asserted  that  logical  procedures  are 
required  to  order  the  data  of  any  science.  Prior  to  these  formal  opera- 
tions, however,  comes  the  issue  of  collecting  the  data.  This  includes 
observing  the  raw  events  and  performing  some  kind  of  discrimination 
or  measurement.  Empirical  rules  are  required  for  this  aspect  of  scien- 
tific activity.  The  interpersonal  system  of  personality  has  attempted 
to  follow  three  commonly  accepted  rules  of  scientific  activity  which 
can  be  formalized  in  a  general  working  principle. 

Eighth  working  principle:  Measurements  of  interpersonal  be- 
havior Tnust  be  public  and  verifiable  operations;  the  variables  must  be 
capable  of  operational  definition.  Our  conclusions  about  human 
nature  cannot  be  presented  as  absolute  facts  but  as  probability  state- 
ments. 

Personality  Variables  Must  Be  Public  and  Verifiable 

The  first  criterion  of  scientific  activity  insists  that  it  must  be  public 
and  verifiable.  Any  statement  we  make  about  the  world  of  events 
must  be  subject  to  independent  check.  Its  validity  eventually  rests  on 
its  confirmation  by  other  scientists.  While  this  social  criterion  of 
knowledge  has  engendered  some  qualifying  controversy  in  the  phi- 
losophy of  the  physical  sciences,  its  employment  in  personality  psy- 
chology at  the  present  time  is  particularly  necessary. 

Psychology,  more  than  any  other  modem  discipline,  has  been 
hampered  by  the  issue  of  "private"  observation.  Many  respectable 
scholars  have  flatly  rejected  the  public  testability  principle  and  have 
endorsed  a  discipline  of  introspection,  intuition,  and  anarchic  indi- 
viduality. Many  brilliant  clinicians  still  stick  by  the  principle  that  the 
human  being  is  a  unique  and  rather  sacred  pattern  of  individuality 

45 


46  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

and  that  any  attempt  to  find  lawful  generality  is  futile,  insulting,  and 
vaguely  inhuman. 

The  patient-oriented  approach  of  the  practitioner  is  highly  credit- 
able, and  needs  no  defense.  To  the  clinician,  the  only  principle  in- 
volved is  the  welfare  of  the  patient.  There  is,  however,  another 
important  aspect  to  this  question.  Our  technical  competence  to  serve 
a  patient  is  limited  to  our  generalized,  probabilistic  lawful  knowledge 
of  human  nature.  Good  will  and  patient-oriented  solicitousness  are 
virtues,  but  they  are  not  professional  instruments.  Many  skillful 
clinicians  overlook  the  fact  that  they  carry  around  inside  of  themselves 
a  complex  set  of  unverbalized  and  often  unconscious  generalizations 
about  human  behavior,  which  they  apply  to  cases.  Their  patients  get 
the  benefit  of  an  unsystematized  lawful  wisdom.  These  principles  are 
often  uncommunicable,  unorganized,  unreachable,  untestable.  They 
produce  nothing  toward  the  broad  social  goal  of  a  science  of  human 
nature. 

The  integrity  and  productivity  of  good  clinicians,  however,  more 
than  justifies  their  unilateral  approach  at  this  primitive  stage  in  the 
field.  They  violate  no  scientific  canons  because  they  do  not  pose  as 
scientists. 

As  soon  as  a  clinician  begins  to  lecture  or  write  about  principles  of 
personality,  however,  he  puts  himself  into  the  area  of  discourse  that 
must  be  bound  by  the  laws  of  scientific  evidence.  The  first  of  these 
necessary  conventions  is  that  the  events,  the  data,  be  open  for  inde- 
pendent verification  by  other  scientists. 

There  is  a  necessary  objection  which  holds  that  psychotherapy  can- 
not be  studied  objectively  because  the  crucial  events — the  interpreta- 
tion, the  instant  resistance  of  the  patient,  etc. — cannot  be  repeated. 
This  comment  is  quite  beside  the  point.  The  data  of  personality  are 
communications  about  human  behavior — descriptions  of  the  subject 
by  himself  and  by  others.  The  reliability  and  verifiability  of  these  can 
be  established  by  means  of  the  most  basic  recording  or  data-preserving 
devices.  The  attempt  to  derive  generalizations  about  human-person- 
ality-in-therapy  probably  will  involve  the  use  of  objective  electric 
recordings  of  the  therapy  process. 

With  simple  devices  of  this  sort,  it  is  possible  to  have  any  number 
of  independent  experts  repeat  and  verify  the  most  complex  variable 
measurements.  Without  them  psychotherapy  becomes  a  wise  but  un- 
communicable art.  WTien  it  becomes  clear  that  the  unit  of  per- 
sonality or  interaction  is  the  discriminatory  element  or  variable,  it  also 
becomes  quite  feasible  to  obtain  any  number  of  equivalent  repetitions 
of  the  variable  by  increasing  the  sample  of  subjects  or  of  future  obser- 
vations. While  it  is  true  that  any  raw  personality  expression  is  unique 


EMPIRICAL  PRINCIPLES 


47 


and  unrepeatable,  the  basic  variable  units  by  means  of  which  we  clas- 
sify behavior  are,  by  definition,  general,  recurrent,  and  verifiable. 

Operational  Definitions  of  Terms 

A  second  and  related  aspect  of  scientific  method  which  holds  for 
personality  psychology  is  that  of  operationism.  This  principle  requires 
that  terms  be  defined  by  the  empirical  operations  which  produce 
them.  In  the  words  of  Bridgman,  "We  mean  by  any  concept  nothing 
more  than  a  set  of  operations."  The  relationship  between  the  terms 
we  use  and  the  empirical  operations  by  which  we  discriminate  them 
must  be  direct  and  openly  expressed. 

In  philosophy,  the  healthy  impact  of  the  operational  definition  has 
been  to  sweep  away  many  metaphysical  pseudoempirical  concepts  for 
which  no  external  reference  existed.  In  psychology  many  terms  which 
have  had  dubious  speculative  histories  have  taken  on  new  objective 
significance  as  researchers  have  linked  their  meaning  to  the  empirical 
procedures  by  which  they  were  measured.  In  personality  and  psycho- 
analytic theory — fields  where  undefined  or  privately  defined  concepts 
flourish  like  jungle  growth — much  less  operational  redefinition  has 
occurred. 

There  can  be  many  operational  definitions  of  the  same  concept. 
Each  scientist  may  find  it  necessary  to  use  different  sets  of  data  to 
define,  for  example,  unconsciousness.  One  may  use  dreams.  Another 
may  employ  fantasy  stories,  and  another,  slips  of  the  tongue.  As  long 
as  each  worker  clearly  states  the  classificatory  operations  to  which  he 
relates  his  term  there  is  no  objection  to  the  individual  differences  in 
approach.  The  rest  of  his  colleagues  are  free  to  accept  or  reject  his 
theories,  but  they  cannot  deny  the  empirical  adequacy  of  his  approach. 

Now,  this  flexibility  of  the  definition  process  is  not  cause  for  alarm, 
nor  is  it  a  sign  of  any  peculiar  looseness  of  the  personality  field.  The 
vahdity  and  meaning  of  any  scientific  fact  is  never  exact  or  final.  It 
always  depends,  among  other  things,  on  the  type  and  level  of  the 
measurement  methods  involved.  Only  metaphysics  can  claim  the 
luxury  of  finality  and  complete  unambiguity.  As  the  philosophers  of 
operationism  have  pointed  out,  there  are  many  ways  to  measure  dis- 
tance— a  yardstick,  a  mileage  indicator,  a  transit  reading.  Each  of 
these  can  be  valid  in  its  own  area  of  discourse.  Many  of  them  can  be 
combined  into  the  same  classification.  Many  cannot,  at  this  point. 
Similarly  our  illustrative  operational  definitions  of  unconsciousness  are 
(to  the  extent  that  they  are  independently  confirmed)  all  valid.  Many 
of  them  may  be  combined.  It  might,  perhaps,  be  determined  that 
dreams  and  fantasy  stories  tap  the  same  level  of  unconsciousness,  and 
allow  a  broader  combined  definition  of  unconsciousness.  Slips  of  the 


48  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

tongue,  possibly,  might  not  be  so  related,  and  therefore  would  define 
another  level  of  unconsciousness  with  its  own  particular  lawful  pre- 
dictiveness. 

The  concepts  of  operationism  have  added  powerful  synthetic  tools 
to  the  scientific  method.  Operational  definitions  have  a  remarkable 
capacity  for  ridding  the  language  of  any  discipline  of  broad,  impres- 
sive, but  empty,  terms  which  have  no  empirical  meaning.  Applied  to 
the  terminology  of  psychiatry,  operationism  calls  for  the  elimination 
or  systematic  redefinition  of  almost  every  current  concept.  Operation- 
ism's  "radical  implications  for  psychiatric  theory  and  practice"  have 
been  programmatically  cited  by  MuUahy.  He  believes  that  "there  is 
no  chance  that  psychiatry  will  ever  be  a  truly  scientific  field  of  inquiry 
until,  as  a  first  step  towards  scientific  progress,  it  adopts  a  language 
sufficiently  precise  that  its  practitioners  as  well  as  workers  in  allied 
and  related  fields  can  in  various  ways  check  and  verify  the  correct- 
ness of  statements  made  by  one  another."  ( 1,  p.  58) 

The  Probability  Nature  of  Predictive  Accuracy 

There  is  a  third  empirical  principle  which  has  importance  for  per- 
sonality psychology.  This  has  to  do  with  the  ultimate  validity  of  em- 
pirical knowledge.  It  holds  that  there  is  no  absolute  or  final  truth, 
that  scientific  laws  are  never  completely  accurate,  and  that  the  only 
knowledge  we  can  have  of  the  empirical  world  is  probable  knowledge. 
The  essence  of  scientific  explanation  is  the  known  relative  accuracy 
of  predictions. 

We  tread  here  on  the  most  ancient  and  hallowed  ground  of  West- 
em  philosophy — epistemological  questions  about  the  validity  of 
knowledge.  Within  the  last  century  statistical  mathematics,  post- 
Newtonian  physics,  and  the  operational  logicians  have  produced  con- 
verging solutions  that  are  closely  related  to  the  needs  and  complexities 
of  a  functionalistic  personality  psychology. 

The  most  accurate  statement  any  scientist  can  make  about  the 
world  of  events  is  an  indication  of  the  probability  of  occurrence. 
The  chances  are,  let  us  say,  three  to  five  that  a  certain  patient  will 
develop  passive  resistance  to  a  male  therapist.  But  the  chances  are 
also  two  to  five  that  he  will  not.  Or  we  might  determine  that  two 
thirds  of  the  patients  with  duodenal  ulcers  will  deny  feelings  of 
passivity  and  weakness.  Of  the  one  third  who  do  not,  80  per  cent 
manifest  another  specific  interpersonal  behavior — most  likely  schizoid 
withdrawal.  When  we  have  accumulated  thousands  of  probability 
figures  of  this  sort,  based  on  publicly  managed  variable  systems  and 
organized  into  multilevel  conceptual  systems,  a  scientific  structure  of 
personality  facts  will  have  been  established.  Predictive  procedures  of 


EMPIRICAL  PRINCIPLES 


49 


limited  but  known  accuracy  will  be  at  hand.  Moreover,  the  com- 
plexity and  variety  of  human  nature  need  never  be  threatened  by  the 
necessary  oversimplifications  of  our  predictive  structures.  There  can 
be  as  many  different  systems  as  there  are  different  dimensions  of  per- 
sonality or  of  facets  to  the  interreacting  environment.  The  system  de- 
scribed in  this  book  is  one  such  conceptual  apparatus.  It  is  designed 
to  make  factual  predictions  about  the  interpersonal  dimension  of  be- 
havior in  the  clinical  situation.  This  is  really  a  very  narrow  slice  of 
the  wide  and  varied  expanse  of  human  behavior.  Other  systems  will 
continue  to  appear.  New  variables  will  be  developed.  Broader  areas 
of  human  behavior  will  be  encompassed  and  integrated.  The  essence 
of  scientific  activity  is  that  new  theories,  new  facts  never  push  out  the 
old.  They  add,  they  revise,  they  refine,  they  expand. 

Thus  we  shall  in  later  chapters  present  operational  definitions  of 
several  psychiatric  and  personality  variables  and  probability  state- 
ments about  their  application.  But  no  note  of  finality  will  be  sounded. 
Future  theorists  will  unquestionably  present  different  and  more  effec- 
tive definitions  of  the  same  concepts — based  on  different  operations 
and  boasting,  perhaps,  higher  probability  relationships  to  functional 
criteria.  To  the  extent  that  these  varying  approaches  are  objective — 
communicable  and  operationally  grounded — the  new  findings  will  not 
disprove  nor  quarrel  with  the  old.  No  scientific  fact  can  be  disproved. 
It  can  be  reinterpreted,  qualified  by  new  relationships,  amplified  to  fit 
new  material.  Scientific  findings  do  not  compete,  debate,  or  attack 
each  other.  They  add,  expand,  and  collaborate  to  develop  new 
hypotheses.  This  characteristic  of  the  scientific  method  is  particularly 
important  in  the  study  of  human  nature  and  has  been  often  neglected. 

Reference 

%.   MuLLAHY,  P.  The  theories  of  H.  S.  Sullivan.  In  P.  Mullahy  (ed.).  The  contribu- 
tions of  Harry  Stack  Sullivan.   New  York:  Hermitage  House,  1952. 


Functional  Theory  of  Personality 


The  preceding  four  chapters  have  presented  a  sequence  of  principles 
which  serve  as  background  to  a  science  of  human  nature.  This  chapter 
discusses  the  functional  purpose  of  scientific  knovi^ledge  in  general  and 
psychological  knowledge  in  particular.  In  so  doing  it  calls  upon  and 
offers  some  synthesis  of  the  principles  already  presented.  There  is 
more  speculation  and  value  orientation  than  in  the  preceding  chapter. 

The  Aims  of  General  Science 

The  ultimate  objective  of  scientific  activity  is  to  explain  and  pre- 
dict. To  control,  change,  cure,  and  improve  are  worthy  motives. 
These  latter  tasks  fall,  however,  within  the  province  of  the  applied 
professions — engineering,  administration,  medicine,  psychiatry.  The 
job  of  the  scientist  is  to  explain  as  accurately  and  as  completely  as 
possible  the  relationships  among  variables  and  to  predict  future  events. 

We  explain  any  event  by  determining  the  probability  relationships 
it  has  with  other  events.  Increasing  the  temperature  above  a  certain 
point  is  related  to  the  boiling  of  water.  Relationships  of  this  sort  in 
the  macroscopic  physical  world  have  such  regularity  that  extremely 
high  predictability  or  exceptionless  cause-effect  sequences  are  gen- 
erally observed.  The  fields  of  atomic  and  subatomic  physics  and  of 
human  behavior  involve  such  a  multiplicity  of  interacting  events  that 
deterministic  causal  laws  are  not  possible  and  probability  statistics 
define  the  order  of  relationship.  "The  more  rejecting  the  parents  are, 
the  higher  expectation  that  the  child  will  manifest  a  defensive  sus- 
piciousness." Did  the  parents'  rejection  cause  the  child's  distrust?  It 
is  much  preferable  to  say  that  the  two  are  correlated  to  a  specific 
degree. 

Probability  laws  allow  us  to  make  generalizations  of  known  ac- 
curacy about  the  subject  matter.  Many  established  relationships  among 
variables  allow  an  increasingly  higher  order  of  generality.  The  breadth 
and  sharpness  of  the  explanatory  process  grow. 

50 


FUNCTIONAL  THEORY  OF  PERSONALITY  5 1 

But  why  do  scientists  attempt  to  explain  natural  and  psychological 
events'  What  is  the  function  of  the  generalized  knowledge  so  ac- 
cumulated? These  questions  lead  us  to  the  other  aim  of  scientific 
activity — prediction. 

The  purpose  of  scientific  explanation  is  to  predict  functionally 
useful  events  of  the  future.  This  conception  of  the  scientist's  role 
(which  is,  by  the  way,  an  opinion  rather  than  an  axiom)  is  a  human- 
istic one.  It  assigns  his  social  function  in  response  to  social  demands 
and  sees  him  as  a  human  being  always  stimulated  by  and  limited  to 
cultural  pressures. 

It  is  interesting  to  speculate  that  the  human  quest  for  knowledge 
has  been  strongly  related  to  man's  motivation  to  know  the  future. 
Knowledge  of  things  to  come  has  an  enormous  and  obvious  survival 
value.  A  major  proportion  of  man's  cognitive,  philosophic  activity  is 
tied  to  his  desire  to  anticipate  correctly  the  future.  Every  religious 
interpretation  has  had  to  rest  its  dogma  on  a  forecast  about  the  nature 
of  an  afterlife.  Much  of  its  irrational  and  powerful  appeal  rests  on 
this  function.  The  interpersonal  counterpart  of  these  speculations 
might  hold  that  ignorance  is  experienced  as  weakness,  helplessness,  and 
survivally  dangerous.  Knowledge  is  experienced  as  mastery  and  au- 
tonomy. It  is  survivally  crucial  in  its  function  of  forecasting  the 
future. 

The  time-bound  essence  of  human  life  requires  that  man  anticipate 
the  things  to  come  with  reasonable  accuracy.  Science  as  the  broad 
branch  of  human  activity  entrusted  with  the  development  and  classi- 
fication of  knowledge  accepts  the  function  of  prediction. 

An  activity  often  erroneously  assigned  to  scientific  activity  is  the 
function  of  control.  Ideally  there  should  be  no  reason  why  the  ap- 
plication of  pertinent  knowledge  to  human  problems  should  not  be 
accomplished  by  the  scientists  who  derive  it.  In  actuality,  the  inter- 
personal behavior  of  human  beings — particularly  along  the  power  axis 
— is  so  corruptible  that  there  is  good  reason  for  the  division  of  labor. 
Objective,  effective  scientific  activity  apparently  suffers  in  direct  pro- 
portion to  the  intensity  of  the  interpersonal  network  involved. 

It  is,  thus,  the  task  of  the  applied  disciplines  to  use  the  predictive 
facts  accruing  from  science.  This  distinction  is  not  an  invidious  one. 
The  years  of  technical  training  involved  in  the  service  professions — 
medicine,  engineering — is  often  as  great  as  or  greater  than  that  of  the 
scientist.  The  responsibilities  undertaken  are  invariably  larger.  So  are 
the  salaries. 

Neither  is  this  distinction  absolute.  Most  researchers  employed  by 
nonacademic  institutions — whether  industries  or  clinics — are  generally 
forced  to  play  a  double  role.  They  follow  their  scientific  noses  and 


^2  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

are  also  led  by  them.  This  collaboration  of  the  scientific  with  the 
applied  is  generally  a  fortunate  one.  Certainly  for  the  problems  of 
psychotherapy  and  personality  change  it  is  hard  to  see  how  much  can 
be  accomplished  without  complete  clinical  training  as  a  minimum  and 
considerable  clinical  practice  as  an  optimum. 

Functional  Theory  of  Personality 

To  this  point  we  have  examined  the  functions  of  science  in  general. 
Turning  to  personality  psychology  we  have  seen  the  objective  of  this 
field  to  explain  and  predict  interpersonal  behavior. 

Objective  empirical  methods  provide  innumerable  probability  rela- 
tionships among  specific  variables.  Formal  and  theoretical  structures 
suggest  how  these  are  to  be  further  related.  This  procedure  poses  new 
hypothetical  questions.  These  are  tested  by  additional  empirical  facts. 
This  reciprocal  progression  of  finding  and  theory  establishes  an  in- 
creasing number  of  factual  clusters  which  themselves  become  related 
to  higher  level  theories. 

As  understanding  grows,  the  predictive  power  of  the  science  be- 
comes more  accurate  and  extensive.  The  functional  importance  of  the 
field  grows,  usually  encouraging  new  cycles  of  empirical  activity. 

The  complexity  of  human  nature  is  such  that  there  are  countless 
facets  of  behavioral  data  and  an  equal  number  of  empirical  problems. 
The  conceptualization  and  terminology  of  the  field  clearly  depend  on 
which  of  these  aspects  of  personality  are  studied.  The  psychologist 
who  spends  all  of  his  time  measuring  and  relating  variables  of  energy 
level  will  generally  develop  terms  and  theories  that  have  something 
to  do  with  energy.  Even  when  we  define  personality  in  terms  of  the 
interpersonal  behaviors,  a  broad  scope  remains.  Every  individual  has 
been  in  crucial  interaction  with  others  since  the  day  of  his  birth,  and 
his  history  of  past  relationships  is  rich.  Concentrating  on  the  present 
rather  than  the  past,  we  see  an  enormously  extensive  network  of  inter- 
personal reactions.  Relationships  in  the  family  situation,  in  the  job 
situation,  or  in  the  social  sphere  all  have  some  explanatory  value.  In 
attempting  to  predict,  which  facet  of  social  behavior  should  be  focused 
on?  We  might  be  able  to  predict  the  interpersonal  consequences  of  a 
subject's  marriage  to  this  girl,  of  his  election  to  that  office  in  the 
Masonic  Lodge,  or  of  the  selection  of  a  certain  program  of  psycho- 
therapy in  the  clinic.  The  relevance  of  the  prediction  clearly  refers 
to  the  problem  being  posed  or  the  questions  being  asked.  Prognostic 
knowledge  is  generally  of  value  to  the  extent  that  it  is  relevant  to  the 
human  problems  at  issue.  To  go  further,  it  is  most  functional  when  the 
variables  and  terminologies  of  explanation  are  directly  related  to,  or 


FUNCTIONAL  THEORY  OF  PERSONALITY  53 

even  in  terms  of,  the  functionally  important  activities.  For  clinical 
psychiatry  this  means  that  the  variable  language  should  refer  most 
directly  to  the  interpersonal  interactions  that  determine  a  successful  or 
unsuccessful  clinical  relationship.  This  point  brings  us  to  the  question 
of  functional  diagnosis  and  deserves  further  illustration. 

Functional  Diagnosis 

Let  us  suppose  that  a  psychotherapist  comes  to  the  predictive  diag- 
nostician posing  this  narrowly  defined  hypothetical  problem.  "In  my 
office  there  is  a  male  adult  patient  with  asthma;  what  predictive  state- 
ments can  you  make?"  By  studying  the  accumulated  generalizations  at 
hand  the  diagnostician  might  make  any  number  of  predictions.  He 
might  report,  "The  chances  are  better  than  two  to  one  that  your 
patient  is  married."  This  interpersonal  prediction  could  be  based  on 
testable  evidence,  but  it  has  little  relevance  to  the  situation  at  hand  and 
little  functional  meaning.  The  diagnostician  might  report,  "The 
chances  are  better  than  two  to  one  that  any  asthmatic  condition  is 
related  to  psychogenic  factors  and  is  therefore  psychosomatic."  This 
is  a  descriptive,  nosological  statement.  It  has  some  relevance  in  that 
the  psychiatrist  can  continue  his  clinical  procedures  with  better  than 
average  chance  that  a  psychological  problem  is  related.  It  certainly 
does  not  throw  much  further  specific  light  on  the  problem. 

A  third  possible  answer  might  be,  "Over  60  per  cent  of  these 
patients  during  childhood  show  marked  ambivalence  toward  the 
maternal  figure  and  intense  oedipal  conflict  with  the  father."  This  his- 
torical explanation  is  clearly  more  pertinent  to  the  understanding  of 
the  patient.  It  might  lead  to  extrapolating  conjectures  from  the  past  to 
the  future,  and  might  assist  in  clarifying  this  patient's  relationship  to 
others  including  the  future  therapist. 

A  fourth  illustrative  forecast  might  state  that  "Over  6$  per 
cent  of  asthmatic  patients  tend  to  be  compulsively  orderly  and  punc- 
tual." This  is  a  testable  psychological  statement  relating  to  the  present, 
but  it  is  molecular  and  peripheral,  and  has  limited  practical  meaning. 
It  is  not  directly  interpersonal.  It  refers  to  stylistic  symptoms  rather 
than  crucial  purposive  direction. 

None  of  these  illustrative  answers  is  adequately  functional.  They 
all  can  be  true.  They  all  might  have  some  relationship  to  the  per- 
sonality organization  of  the  patient,  but  their  bearing  on  the  situation 
is  not  central.  The  pressure  of  the  human  problem  at  stake  is  not 
effectively  met  by  these  statements.  In  the  clinical  situation,  a  gen- 
eralized statement  is  most  relevant  to  the  extent  that  it  predicts  the 
future  course  of  clinical  progress.    A  diagnostic  statement  about  a 


54 


BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 


psychiatric  patient  is  most  functional  to  the  extent  that  it  forecasts 
interpersonal  behavior  pertinent  to  the  therapeutic  handling  of  his 
problem. 

The  patient  cannot  change  his  childhood  experience,  although  it  is 
very  true  that  he  can  learn  from  it.  The  historical  prediction  is,  thus, 
valuable,  but  not  crucial.  Nor  is  the  patient's  situation  very  dependent 
on  diagnosing  him  psychosomatic.  The  diagnostic  label  is  made  by 
and  is  important  to  the  clinician,  and  not  to  the  patient.  Changing  this 
descriptive  term  would  have  very  little  effect  on  the  symptom  or  the 
underlying  character  structure.  Neither  does  the  symptomatic  molec- 
ular prediction  about  compulsive  orderiiness  have  central  importance. 
The  punctuality  and  neatness  are  undoubtedly  related  to  basic  inter- 
personal motivations,  but  to  focus  on  them  diagnostically  or  thera- 
peutically would  not  be  a  recommended  course  of  action.  These 
stylistic  "how"  variables  of  personality  take  on  their  vital  meaning 
when  they  are  traced  back  to  the  interpersonal  purposes  which  they 
serve.  To  change  just  the  peripheral,  noninterpersonal  trait  is  not  the 
essence  of  therapeutic  improvement. 

The  most  functional  answer  to  the  clinician's  question  might  go 
like  this,  "Over  75  per  cent  of  male  asthmatic  patients  who  come  to  a 
psychiatric  clinic  manifest  autonomous  and  stubborn  competitiveness 
with  males  of  superior  or  equal  status.  Conscious  awareness  of  this 
intense  fear  of  weakness  is  generally  followed  by  overt  signs  of  severe 
anxiety  and  increased  competitive  behavior.  The  chances  are  three  to 
two  that  these  patients  will  interrupt  therapy  in  autonomous  resist- 
ance." This  prediction  serves  to  illustrate  the  issues  of  relevant  predic- 
tion and  functional  diagnosis.^ 

A  statement  of  this  sort  is  preferable  for  several  reasons.  It  is  inter- 
personal. It  relates  to  the  future;  not  just  to  one  expected  event,  but  to 
a  sequence  of  interaction  (which  is  related  to  a  conflict  between  levels 
of  personality).  It  relates  the  expected  interpersonal  pattern  to  an 
estimate  of  treatability.  The  diagnostic  concepts  are  expressed  directly 
in  terms  of  predictive  behavior  which  has  bearing  on  the  future  treat- 
ment relationship.  The  future  therapist  is  told  specifically  how  the 
patient  might  be  expected  to  react  to  the  therapist  and  to  the  treatment 
process.  His  attention  is  directed  to  the  interpersonal  responses  which 
have  so  much  to  do  with  the  success  or  failure  of  the  therapy  plan. 

This  last  is  an  interesting  sidelight  of  functional  terminology.  The 
predictive  terms  that  a  diagnostic  system  employs  not  only  reflect  its 
theoretical  focus.  They  also  exercise  a  subtle  but  marked  effect  on  the 
subsequent  use  made  of  the  information.  If  a  theoretical  system  (and 

*  See  Appendix  D  for  an  illustration  of  a  personality  report  employing  the  inter- 
personal system  to  make  a  practical  prediction  about  a  patient's  behavior  in  the  clinic. 


FUNCTIONAL  THEORY  OF  PERSONALITY 


55 


the  diagnostic  terms  it  sponsors)  emphasizes  past  events  of  the  case 
history,  it  is  likely  that  the  following  discussions  will  tend  to  em- 
phasize these  areas.  If  the  predictions  in  the  hypothetical  case  employ 
the  language  of  compulsivity,  punctuality,  and  the  like,  the  facets  of 
behavior  may  be  unduly  attended  to  in  the  interviews  that  follow. 
Suggestibility  and  selectivity  of  content  cues  are  the  constant  errors  of 
psychotherapy.  The  less  experienced  or  the  less  flexible  the  therapist, 
the  more  influence  accruing  to  this  indoctrinating  effect  of  diagnostic 
terms. 

In  clinical  practice  we  assess  the  functional  value  of  a  personality 
or  psychiatric  variable  in  terms  of  the  predictive  value  for  facilitating 
the  future  clinical  relationship.  Terms  which  have  high  predictive 
value  (even  if  indirect)  tend  to  remain  in  popular  use.  Terms  which 
have  little  predictive  "cash  value"  tend  to  disappear.  Every  psy- 
chiatric term  possesses  a  cluster  of  prognostic  nuances  which  influence 
the  intake  and  therapeutic  diagnosis.  Most  of  these  predictive  at- 
tributes are  vague,  unproven,  often  implicit,  but  they  carry  a  stagger- 
ing load  of  responsibility. 

Schizophrenia,  for  example,  brings  to  mind  a  host  of  prognostic 
associations,  "not  a  good  outpatient,"  "poor  risk  for  brief  therapy," 
"poor  risk  for  psychoanalysis,"  "supportive  or  ego-strengthening 
methods  favored,"  "long  institutional  treatment  optimal,"  "generally 
slow  prognosis,"  etc.  These  distillations  of  clinical  wisdom  are  un- 
systematized, unverified  probability  statements  about  the  future  be- 
havior of  schizophrenic  patients.  The  original  diagnosis  is  presumably 
based  on  other  classes  of  variable  cues.  That  is,  the  patient  is  originally 
diagnosed  schizophrenic  because  of  delusions,  withdrawal,  marked 
projections  on  or  misperceptions  of  reality,  and  the  like.  Some  psy- 
chiatrists hold  that  the  best  diagnostic  sign  indicating  poor  prognosis 
is  the  elicitation  of  hallucinatory  material. 

This  type  of  informal  cUnical  folklore  is  a  necessary  and  healthy 
development  in  an  infant  field.  The  criteria  of  prognostic  value  (how- 
ever vague  the  variable  relationships)  indicate  that  the  discipline  is 
struggling  toward  a  predictive  status.  As  this  process  occurs  the  usage 
of  certain  terms  with  lesser  prognostic  power  begins  to  diminish.  They 
maintain  only  descriptive  and  administrative  popularity.  Hebephrenic 
is  such  a  term.  Outside  of  some  crude  differentiations  from  the  folk- 
lore of  the  shock  ward  there  is  little  prognostic  specificity  which  dis- 
tinguishes this  term  from,  let  us  say,  catatonic. 

The  most  functionally  important  aspects  of  human  behavior  seem 
to  be  the  interpersonal.  To  understand  a  human  being  is  to  have  proba- 
bility evidence  about  his  relationships  with  others  (perceived,  actual, 
or  symbolic),  about  the  durable  interpersonal  techniques  by  which  he 


$6 


BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 


wards  off  anxiety,  and  about  the  reciprocal  responses  these  techniques 
pull  from  others.  To  make  meaningful  predictions  about  a  human 
being  is  to  translate  our  explanatory  data  into  statements  as  to  the 
expected  interpersonal  behaviors  in  specific  functional  situations. 

Explanatory  concepts  which  deal  with  instincts,  body  apertures, 
symptomatic  manifestations,  and  peripheral  stylistic  traits  have  in- 
direct value  to  the  extent  that  they  can  be  related  to  interpersonal 
behaviors.  It  is  not  really  of  much  use  to  a  future  therapist  to  predict 
that  his  patient  will  be  punctual  and  not  flick  ashes  on  the  rug. 

It  seems  quite  possible  that  within  a  few  decades  the  slowly 
evolving  laws  of  pragmatic  usage  will  establish  interpersonal  concepts 
as  a  popular  and  useful  diagnostic  language.  Two  possibilities  suggest 
themselves  here — the  first  is  that  direct  interpersonal  terms  will  replace 
the  disorganized  nosology  of  present-day  psychiatry;  the  second  is 
that  the  current  terms  will  be  redefined  in  interpersonal  terms.  If  the 
first  alternative  is  accepted,  terms  such  as  psychopathic  personality  or 
schizoid  personality  would  disappear  in  favor  of  specific  systematic 
interpersonal  labels.  According  to  the  second  alternative,  psychopathic 
personality  would  be  redefined  operationally  in  terms  of  the  rebel- 
lious aggressive  criteria,  and  schizoid  personality  would  have  as  its 
basic  diagnostic  indices  distrust  and  bitter  withdrawal.  This  is  another 
historical  issue  that  time  will  settle. 

The  system  described  in  this  book  employs  the  latter — more  con- 
servative— solution  for  developing  a  functional,  operationally  defined 
language  of  personahty  which  will  work  for  both  adaptive  adjustment 
and  the  psychiatric  extremes. 

Functional  Co?icept  of  Personality 

Two  general  postulates,  which  have  been  woven  in  as  background 
for  all  of  the  discussions  so  far,  hold  that  the  functional  core  of  human 
behavior  is  the  interpersonal,  and  that  personality  concepts  must  be 
defined  along  adjustment  continua  which  include  both  normal  and 
abnormal  reactions.  When  we  approach  the  problem  of  a  functional 
personality  language  with  these  two  principles  in  mind,  certain  solu- 
tions seem  to  follow  quite  readily. 

The  first  assumption  clearly  demands  that  the  basic  set  of  personal- 
ity variables  be  not  symptomatic,  erotogenic,  or  stylistic,  but  inter- 
personal. The  second  assumption  suggests  that  each  of  these  variables 
must  have  an  intensity  dimension  such  that  its  rigid,  maladaptive  ex- 
treme be  as  readily  classified  as  its  moderate  adaptive  aspect.  The 
measurement  categories  all  along  this  scale  are  still  interpersonal — as 
we  recall  from  the  hostility  continuum  described  in  Chapter  2,  where 
blunt,  frank,  appropriately  critical  were  terms  referring  to  the  adaptive 


FUNCTIONAL  THEORY  OF  PERSONALITY  57 

and  sadistic,  aggressive  to  the  maladaptive  end  of  the  continuum.  Now 
it  is  well  known  that  the  language  of  psychiatry  deals  almost  ex- 
clusively with  the  pathological  extreme  of  behavior.  Thus  we  dis- 
cover that  maladaptive  extremities  of  the  continuum  for  each  generic 
interpersonal  motivation  are  most  closely  related  to  and  overlap  the 
psychiatric.  In  the  illustration  of  the  hostility  continuum  just  men- 
tioned, it  will  be  noted  that  sadistic  and  aggressive  have  a  much  more 
psychiatric  flavor  than  do  blunt,  frank,  appropriately  critical,  and 
the  like. 

It  seems  to  follow,  then,  that  if  we  painstakingly  study  all  the  forms 
of  interpersonal  behavior  in  as  many  environmental  situations  as  pos- 
sible, we  shall  obtain,  after  grouping  and  sifting,  a  finite  number  of 
discernible  basic  interpersonal  motivations  all  of  which  must  (ac- 
cording to  the  normality  assumption)  be  placed  on  adaptive-mal- 
adaptive  continua.  For  each  pathological  interpersonal  pattern  we  ob- 
serve in  the  clinic  there  must  be  an  adjustive  aspect.  And  for  each 
successful  social  maneuver  we  meet  in  the  market  place  there  must 
be  a  pathological  extreme.  The  surprising  linguistic  imbalance  which 
implies  that  an  Anglo-Saxon  cannot  be  too  affectionate  or  adaptively 
disaffiliative  has  already  been  commented  upon.  The  implications  of 
this  imbalance  for  systematic  functional  diagnosis  will  be  developed  in 
later  chapters. 

Since  the  neurotic  interpersonal  intensities  tend  to  overlap  some 
aspects  of  the  noninterpersonal  psychiatric  categories,  we  have  close 
to  hand  a  solution  for  the  problem  of  what  to  do  with  these  latter  less 
functional  terms.  The  process  of  redefining  them  begins  to  take  place 
automatically.  Most  of  the  popular  diagnostic  labels  have  vague,  un- 
defined, but  fairly  effective  functional  power.  They  have  interpersonal 
correlates.  To  be  skeptical,  realistic,  and  reserved  is  generally  an 
adaptive  interpersonal  pattern.  To  be  inflexibly  distrustful  and  with- 
drawn is  invariably  maladjustive.  Many  psychiatrists  would  call  it 
schizoid.  Thus  we  see  the  possibilities  of  redefining  the  classical 
language  of  administrative  psychiatry  in  interpersonal  terms.  This 
preserves  the  usefulness  of  the  older  terminology  while  sharpening 
its  denotive  power.  On  the  other  hand,  from  the  standpoint  of  the 
interpersonal  system  we  have  added  a  new  set  of  partially  interpersonal 
terms  to  our  linguistic  structure  which  is  broadened  thereby.  The  ex- 
treme points  of  the  scales  now  have  a  new  set  of  descriptive  terms 
which  are  unique  to  the  professional  specialists  of  the  clinic  but  which 
relate  to  the  broader  system  of  general  interpersonal  psychology.  An 
interpersonal  notational  system  holds  the  promise  of  bridging  the  an- 
cient and  logically  intolerable  gap  between  the  science  of  personality 
and  the  practice  of  psychiatry. 


j8  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

There  will  probably  be  many  such  reciprocal  rapprochements  in 
the  next  phases  in  the  study  of  human  nature.  The  scientist  or  systema- 
tist  will  do  well,  we  suggest,  to  keep  his  general  concepts  from  being 
swallowed  up  by  the  more  exciting  linguistics  of  the  clinic.  It  is  most 
valuable  to  stress  the  relationship  between  general  concepts  of  per- 
sonality and  the  terminology  of  the  practitioner.  It  is  important,  how- 
ever, to  maintain  the  basic  nature  of  the  generic  interpersonal  systems. 

If  this  is  done,  the  possibilities  of  relating  the  general  sciences  of 
interpersonal  behavior  with  other  applied  and  pure  disciplines  in  addi- 
tion to  clinical  psychiatry  appear  bright.  There  is,  for  example,  good 
reason  to  feel  that  occupational  adjustment  is  mainly  determined  by 
interpersonal  factors.  Whether  the  applied  field  is  vocational  counsel- 
ing or  industrial  management,  the  terminology  of  job  classification  is 
very  likely  to  have  interpersonal  correlates,  with,  perhaps,  even  more 
overlap  than  psychiatric  labels.  These  vocational  "diagnostic"  terms 
are  most  likely  to  be  located  near  the  adaptive  and  moderate  end  of 
the  normality-abnormality  continua — blunt,  frank,  realistic,  amiable, 
etc.  A  similar  cross-fertilization  and  functional  application  seems 
quite  feasible.  Wherever  an  applied  discipline  requires  psychological 
(not  physiological)  answers  to  the  problems  it  faces,  an  interpersonal 
psychology  will  generally  be  best  equipped  to  make  the  most  basic 
explanations  and  the  most  functional  predictions. 

Thp  Working  Principle  of  Functional  Applicability 

The  functional  orientation  which  has  just  been  described  can  be 
summarized  in  the  form  of  a  guiding  statement. 

Ninth  working  principle:  The  system  of  personality  should  be 
designed  to  measure  behavior  in  the  functional  context  {avhich  in  this 
book  is  the  psychiatric  clinic).  Its  language,  variables,  and  diagnostic 
categories  should  relate  directly  to  the  behavior  expressed  or  to  the 
practical  decisions  to  be  made  in  this  functional  situation.  The  system 
should  yield  predictions  about  interpersonal  behavior  to  be  expected 
in  the  psychiatric  clinic. 


General  Survey  of  Interpersonal 
and  Variability  Systems 


The  preceding  five  chapters  have  presented  a  general,  theoretical 
discussion  of  some  of  the  basic  requirements  of  an  adequate  science  of 
personality. 

By  way  of  summary  the  nine  working  principles  which  have  guided 
the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  in  personality  will  now  be  reviewed 
before  surveying  the  personality  system. 

Nme  Working  Principles  for 

the  Interpersonal  Theory  of  Personality 

(1)  Personality  is  the  multilevel  pattern  of  interpersonal  responses 
(overt,  conscious  or  private)  expressed  by  the  individual.  Interpersonal 
behavior  is  aimed  at  reducing  anxiety.  All  the  social,  emotional,  inter- 
personal activities  of  an  individual  can  be  understood  as  attempts  to  avoid 
anxiety  or  to  establish  and  maintain  self-esteem. 

(2)  The  variables  of  a  personality  system  should  be  designed  to  meas- 
ure—on the  same  continuum— the  normal  or  "adjustive"  aspects  of  behavior 
as  well  as  abnormal  or  pathological  extremes. 

( 3 )  Measurement  of  interpersonal  behavior  requires  a  broad  collection 
of  simple,  specific  variables  which  are  systematically  related  to  each  other 
and  which  are  applicable  to  the  study  of  adjustive  or  maladjustive 
responses. 

(4)  For  each  variable  or  variable  system  by  which  we  measure  the 
subject's  behavior  (at  all  levels  of  personality)  we  must  include  an  equiv- 
alent set  for  measuring  the  behavior  of  specified  "others"  with  whom  the 
subject  interacts. 

(5)  Any  statement  about  personality  must  indicate  the  level  of  per- 
sonality to  which  it  refers. 

(6)  The  levels  of  personality  employed  in  any  theoretical  system  must 
be  specifically  listed  and  defined.  The  formal  relationships  which  exist 
among  the  levels  must  be  outlined.  Once  the  logical  system  of  levels  and 

59 


6o  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

relationships  among  levels  is  defined  it  cannot  be  changed  without  revising 
all  previous  references  to  levels. 

(7)  The  same  variable  system  should  be  employed  to  measure  inter- 
personal behavior  at  all  levels  of  personality, 

(8)  Our  measurements  of  interpersonal  behavior  must  be  public  and 
verifiable  operations;  the  variables  must  be  capable  of  operational  defi- 
nition. Our  conclusions  about  human  nature  cannot  be  presented  as 
absolute  facts  but  as  probability  statements. 

(9)  The  system  of  personality  should  be  designed  to  measure  behavior 
in  a  functional  context  (e.g.,  the  psychiatric  clinic).  Its  language,  variables, 
and  diagnostic  categories  should  relate  directly  to  the  behavior  expressed 
or  to  the  practical  decisions  to  be  made  in  this  functional  situation.  The 
system,  when  used  as  a  clinical  instrument,  should  yield  predictions  about 
interpersonal  behavior  to  be  expected  in  the  psychiatric  clinic  (e.g.,  in 
future  psychotherapy). 

In  the  next  six  chapters  (which  comprise  the  second  section  of  the 
book)  these  postulates  will  be  employed  in  an  attempt  to  construct 
such  a  system.  The  nature  of  these  requirements  tends  to  determine 
and  limit  the  resulting  personality  system.  In  this  chapter  the  over-all 
organization  of  the  personality  system  will  be  described  in  terms  of 
(1)  a  schema  for  classifying  interpersonal  behavior  and  (2)  a  formal 
notational  system  for  defining  and  relating  the  levels  of  personality. 
The  subsequent  chapters  will  focus  respectively  on  five  levels  of  per- 
sonality and  the  way  in  which  they  are  combined  and  used  for  inter- 
personal diagnosis. 

Before  presenting  the  outline  of  the  personality  system,  let  us  illus- 
trate by  way  of  review  the  importance  of  formal  theory  for  dealing 
with  the  levels  of  personality.  Some  remarks  by  the  philosopher 
Reichenbach  (on  the  value  of  symbolic  logic)  may  be  appropriate  in 
this  connection.   He  suggests  that: 

The  introduction  of  a  symbolic  notation  is  important  to  logical  procedure 
because  "it  has  about  the  same  significance  as  a  good  mathematical  notation." 
Suppose  you  are  given  the  problem:  "If  Peter  were  5  years  younger,  he  would 
be  twice  as  old  as  Paul  was  when  he  was  6  years  younger,  and  if  Peter  were 
9  years  older,  he  would  be  thrice  as  old  as  Paul,  if  Paul  were  4  years  younger." 
Try  to  solve  it  in  the  head  by  adding  and  subtracting  and  considering  all  the 
"if's,"  and  you  will  soon  arrive  at  a  sort  of  dizziness  as  though  you  were 
riding  on  a  merry-go-round.  Then  take  a  pen  and  paper,  call  Peter's  age  x  and 
Paul's  age  y,  write  down  the  resulting  equations  and  solve  them  the  way  you 
learned  it  in  high  school— and  you  will  know  what  a  notational  technique  is 
good  for.  There  are  similar  problems  in  logic.  (10,  p.  219) 

There  are  also  similar  problems  in  dynamic  psychology.  Consider 
this  not  atypical  case  report  from  a  psychoanalytic  journal.  The 
author  describes  a  multilevel  pattern  of  the  patient's  emotions  as  fol- 
lows: 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  6i 

While  expressing  aggression  toward  a  male  cousin,  she  thought  once  again 
that  she  smelled  gas.  At  first  by  allusion  to  others,  then  by  way  of  dreams, 
there  emerged  the  fantasy  that  the  analyst  was  feminine;  then  she  admitted 
never  having  thought  of  her  father  as  a  man,  but  as  a  woman. 

She  wished  she  could  dominate  the  analyst  and  others  as  she  felt  dominated 
at  home.  This  aggressive  urge  was  accompanied  by  increased  feelings  of  guilt. 
At  a  time  when  she  had  unconscious  conflicts  about  not  paying  for  cancelled 
hours,  and  also  had  arranged  for  more  advanced  art  lessons  which  would 
increase  her  abilities  and  prestige,  she  stuck  two  fingers  into  an  electric  fan,  and 
was  unable  to  work. 

Seductive  fantasies  toward  the  analyst,  as  well  as  homosexual  dreams  and 
fantasies,  and  dreams  of  being  gassed  and  raped  emerged  in  connection  with 
memories  of  compulsive  masturbation  in  her  childhood,  causing  vaginal  dis- 
charge which  she  had  had  impulses  to  eat.  After  confessing  her  "dirty  thoughts" 
she  had  a  dream. 

"She  stood  before  a  mirror  admiring  herself,  dressed  in  a  beautiful 
flowing  white  dress." 

She  said  this  dress  made  her  look  "effeminate"  and  then  felt  embarrassed  at 
the  use  of  the  word.  She  felt  that  to  be  beautiful  would  serve  two  purposes:  to 
make  her  sister  and  other  girls  feel  inferior  to  her,  and  to  control  men.  She  had 
often  thought  mouth  and  vagina  were  equivalent. 

After  this  dream  she  became  cleaner,  worked  better,  and  began  to  earn  her 
way  both  by  art  work  and  by  working  in  a  department  store.  Competitive 
strivings  in  regard  to  other  patients,  as  well  as  her  sister,  came  out  more  clearly 
in  association  to  wishes  to  be  dirty.  (7,  p.  79) 

If  the  reader  attempts  to  organize  this  series  of  conflicting  events,  to 
sort  out  the  levels  and  the  motives  which  belong  to  them,  ambivalent, 
autistic,  past,  present,  he  may  acquire  a  sort  of  vertigo  similar  to  that 
mentioned  by  Reichenbach. 

This  analyst  has  combined  at  least  four  or  five  levels  of  behavior  in 
this  passage.  He  describes  certain  overt  actions  of  the  patient:  "ex- 
pressing aggression,"  "arranged  for  art  lessons,"  "stuck  two  fingers 
in  a  fan,"  "worked  better,"  "began  to  earn  her  own  way."  All  of 
these  actions  are  public — and  could  be  consensually  validated  by 
listeners  or  observers. 

The  analyst  also  mentions  certain  wishes,  urges,  or  impulses  which 
the  patient  reported:  "to  dominate  the  analyst,"  "to  eat,"  "competitive 
striving."  These  impulses,  consciously  recognized  but  not  acted  out, 
must  be  kept  systematically  distinct  from  the  above-mentioned  overt 
actions. 

Another  level  at  which  this  patient  operates  is  that  of  dream  or 
fantasy:  "that  the  analyst  was  feminine,"  "her  father  as  ...  a 
woman,"  "seductive  fantasies"  toward  the  analyst,  as  well  as  homo- 
sexual dreams  and  fantasies,  and  "dreams  of  being  gassed  and  raped," 
etc.  These  autistic  productions  are  clearly  deeper  or  further  from  real- 
ity than  the  overt  activities  or  the  secret  wishes  previously  summarized. 


(5i  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

To  these  three  levels  we  might  also  add  the  deeper  unconscious 
conflicts  and  the  conscious  reports — both  of  which  denote  different 
orders  of  reality-contact  and  consciousness. 

Free  association  protocols,  case  histories,  and  reports  of  therapeutic 
interaction  comprise  important  sources  of  data  upon  which  the  science 
of  personality  must  be  built.  In  order  to  make  reliable  measurements, 
valid  judgments,  and  meaningful  analyses  the  multilevel  jumble  of 
motivations  which  so  often  characterizes  personality  descriptions  must 
be  organized  into  a  systematic  language. 

In  this  chapter  we  will  describe  first  a  classificatory  system  for  or- 
dering interpersonal  behavior.  Then  we  shall  present  a  notational  sys- 
tem— a  crude  mathematic  or  grammar  of  personality — which  attempts 
to  order  the  levels  of  behavior.  We  shall  present  the  units  or  variables 
by  which  the  behavior  can  be  measured,  and  five  levels  at  which  they 
operate. 

The  classificatory  system  allows  us  to  measure  interpersonal  be- 
havior at  any  of  these  five  levels.  The  notational  schema  defines  the 
levels  and  the  fixed  arithmetic  relationships  among  these  levels.  It 
provides  for  the  diagrammatic  and  numerical  analysis  of  the  personal- 
ity structure. 

The  Classification  System:  The  Interpersonal 
Variables  of  Personality 
In  beginning  the  long  task  of  developing  a  personality  system,  the 
first  assumption  refers  to  the  kind  of  behavior  to  be  studied.  We  have 
defined  this  as  the  interpersonal  core  of  personality.  The  initial  step 
for  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  was,  therefore,  to  focus  on 
this  dimension  of  behavior.  To  this  end  a  wide  assortment  of  raw  in- 
terpersonal data  was  assembled.  Several  scores  of  individuals — male 
and  female,  neurotic,  psychosomatic,  and  normal — were  brought  into 
interpersonal  relationships  in  small  groups.  Some  of  these  were  dis- 
cussion groups  in  a  nonpsychiatric  setting.  Some  were  psychotherapy 
groups  in  an  outpatient  clinic.  The  hundreds  of  interactions  of  each 
subject  were  observed,  recorded,  and  studied.  Many  other  types  of 
interpersonal  behavior  were  obtained  from  the  same  subjects.  Their 
verbal  descriptions  of  self  and  others — present,  past,  and  anticipated 
— as  expressed  in  the  groups  or  as  summarized  in  autobiographies  and 
psychological  inventories  were  collected.  Their  dreams  and  fantasies 
were  recorded.  Their  responses  on  batteries  of  projective  tests  were 
elicited.  A  rich  but  unwieldy  collection  of  raw  materials — in  the  form 
of  wire  recording  spools,  typed  transcriptions,  ratings,  observers'  re- 
ports, test  indices,  projective  responses — piled  up  for  each  subject. 
In  line  with  our  first  theoretical  assumption,  the  interpersonal  aspects 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  63 

of  the  stimulus  material  were  taken  as  the  focus  of  attention.  As  the 
research  team  observed  this  undigested  mass  of  protocol  records  ac- 
cumulating, the  next  research  question  occurred.  How  shall  we 
analyze  these  data?  It  was  clear  that  classiiicatory  assistance  was  re- 
quired. This  came  in  the  form  of  the  second  working  principle,  which 
holds  that  the  basic  data  of  personality  are  not  the  raw  responses  but 
the  units  of  protocol  language  by  which  the  subject's  interpersonal 
behavior  can  be  summarized. 

The  selection  of  this  language,  as  we  have  seen,  has  been  a  crucial 
aspect  of  all  personality  theories.  What  and  how  many  are  the  con- 
ceptual units  of  social  interaction?  The  third  working  principle  en- 
ters at  this  point,  stating  that  measurement  of  interpersonal  behavior 
requires  a  broad  collection  of  simple,  specific  variables  which  are 
applicable  to  the  study  of  adjustive  and  maladjustive  responses. 

With  these  guiding  principles  in  mind,  the  diverse  data  were  studied 
to  determine  the  optimal  number  of  specific  variables  and  their  orderly 
relationship.  As  a  first  step  the  interactions  of  the  subjects  were  studied 
by  three  independent  judges  who  attempted  a  straightforward  verbal 
description  of  the  interpersonal  activity.  In  rating  the  observed  and 
recorded  interactions,  it  was  noticed  that  transitive  verbs  were  the 
handiest  words  for  describing  what  the  subjects  did  to  each  other, 
e.g.,  insult,  challenge,  answer,  help.  In  rating  the  content  of  the  spoken 
or  written  descriptions  of  self-or-other,  it  was  noted  that  adjectives 
were  more  often  suitable.  Here  we  were  interested  in  the  attributes, 
qualities,  and  traits  which  the  subject  assigned  to  himself  and  others. 
"I  am  friendly,  helpful,  strong;  they  are  hostile,  selfish,  wise,  helpful^ 
A  clear  relationship  seemed  to  exist  between  these  two  types  of  inter- 
personal description,  such  that  the  adjectives  seemed  to  express  an 
interpersonal  attribute  or  potentiality  for  action,  while  the  verbs 
described  the  action  directly.  Three  rather  interesting  notions  began 
to  develop  out  of  this  fact.  First,  the  relationships  between  different 
expressions  of  personality  can  be  directly  related  to  each  other  by 
grammatical  or  linguistic  procedures.  That  is,  what  you  actually  do 
in  the  social  situation  as  described  by  a  verb  (e.g.,  help)  can  be  re- 
lated to  your  description  of  yourself  (as  described  by  the  attribute 
helpful)  and  to  your  description  of  your  dream-self  or  fantasy-self 
(also  attributive,  helpful  or  perhaps  unhelpful).  These  grammatical 
relationships  became  the  key  to  a  systematic  consideration  of  the 
levels  of  personality,  of  which  more  later. 

After  extensive  informal  surveys  of  the  many  varieties  of  data,  a 
list  of  several  hundred  terms  for  describing  interpersonal  behavior  was 
assembled.  The  next  task  was  to  sort  through  the  long  lists  of  terms 
and  to  determine  the  generic  interpersonal  motives.   Combining  the 


64  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

action  verbs  with  the  corresponding  attributive  adjectives  cut  down 
the  list.  Thus  the  adjective  insulting  was  subsumed  under  its  action 
category  to  insult.  Next  the  intense  and  statistically  rare  terms  were 
combined  with  the  moderate  and  more  frequent  categories.  For  ex- 
ample, the  themes  of  murder,  attack,  insult,  etc.,  were  included  under 
the  generic  concept  of  hostile  activities.  The  gradually  developing 
lists  of  generic  terms  were  then  combined  to  eliminate  overlaps  and 
repetitions  until  a  list  of  sixteen  generic  interpersonal  motivations  re- 
sulted. All  of  the  original  terms — which  numbered  several  hundred — 
could  be  expressed  as  differentiated  varieties  of  the  sixteen  basic  inter- 
personal themes.  In  this  manner  the  goal  of  breadth,  specificity,  and 
simplicity  was  approached. 

The  principle  of  systematic  relatedness  then  determined  the  next 
task.  This  criterion  demands  that  the  variables  be  ordered  along 
continua  in  such  a  way  that  fixed  relationships  exist  between  the  ele- 
ments. The  question  here  becomes:  What  and  how  many  are  the  di- 
mensions along  which  the  variables  are  to  be  scaled?  In  this  instance, 
it  became  apparent  that  a  two-dimensional  grid  was  optimal  for  re- 
lating the  variables  at  hand.  We  cannot  doubt  that  more  complex  for- 
mal systems  will  eventually  add  new  spatial  dimensions  to  the  organi- 
zation of  personality.  For  the  present,  however,  a  two-dimensional 
space  offers  sufficient  complexity  for  the  data  and  more  than  a;  suf- 
ficient complexity  of  methodological  problems. 

In  surveying  the  list  of  more  or  less  generic  interpersonal  trends, 
it  became  clear  that  they  all  had  some  reference  to  a  power  or  affilia- 
tion factor.  When  dominance-submission  was  taken  as  the  vertical  axis 
and  hostility-affection  as  the  horizontal,  all  of  the  other  generic  inter- 
personal factors  could  be  expressed  as  combinations  of  these  four  nodal 
points.  The  various  types  of  nurturant  behavior  appeared  to  be 
blends  of  strong  and  affectionate  orientations  toward  others.  Dis- 
trustful behaviors  seemed  to  blend  hostility  and  weakness.  Further 
experimentation  and  review  of  the  raw  data  led  to  the  conclusion  that 
a  circular  two-dimensional  continuum  of  sixteen  generic  variables  rep- 
resented the  optimal  degree  of  refinement  of  interpersonal  themes. 
Attempts  at  more  specific  systematization  of  interpersonal  behavior 
by  increasing  the  number  of  variables  led  to  difficulties  in  establishing 
clear  criteria  for  discrimination  between  neighboring  variables.  On 
the  other  hand,  use  of  grosser  units  of  discrimination,  e.g.,  only  the 
four  nodal  variables,  resulted  in  neglect  of  important  shadings  of  inter- 
personal intent. 

The  sixteen  generic  interpersonal  themes  are  presented  in  Figure  1 . 
Each  one  has  been  assigned  a  code  letter.  Thus,  Dominant  behavior 
is  classified  under  the  letter  A,  Autonomous  behavior  under  the  letter 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  65 

B,  etc.  Several  suggestive  terms  are  listed  for  each  generic  type  of 
interpersonal  purpose  in  Figure  1.  Actually,  there  is  an  almost  inex- 
haustible list  of  terms  for  each  generic  code  letter.  The  many  varieties 
of  interpersonal  behavior  included  under  each  category  will  become 
increasingly  clear  as  we  take  up  the  ratings  for  the  different  levels. 


,#^    \VSoc 


DOMINATE 
BOSS, 
ORDER 


PROVOKES 
>(\  OBEDIENCE/ 


<^^^'J' 


<^A^ 


>^1 


^^:^. 


MANAGE, 
DIRECT, 
LEAD 


<;°^. 
<%'■ 


i$i^\^i 


^,^'^^  /,fti 


^\   iL^  •*>.>* 


Q 

LiJ 


Si 


■-v. 


\ 


/V    i\^ 


^. 


% 


■Oft  ■>> 


MM'"'^°' 


ONES 
DUTY, 
OBEV 


^^ 


IrX    \^^ 


^PtJ^/v      /  PROVOKES  ' 
'^/Vv^-'-SL/leaoershipj 


^^^r^::^^)' 


^Cns  ^Cr^'^'^OA,   /  SPINELESS    \0^^o  T\\>^1=.   >^^ 
^Oe^O'Vs'^^/      ACTIONS,      y'^^oOS^O^^^^V^ 

c^^Cj'"  sA  .  /      SUBMIT      \     cO^><;^^' -J/ 


la^-HASOCWm^ 


%^^^ 


Figure  1.  Classificaiion  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  into  Sixteen  Mechanisms  or 
Reflexes.  Each  of  the  sixteen  interpersonal  variables  is  illustrated  by  sample  behaviors 
The  inner  circle  presents  illustrations  of  adaptive  reflexes,  e  g.,  for  the  variable  A, 
manage.  The  center  ring  indicates  the  type  of  behavior  that  this  interpersonal  reflex 
tends  to  "pull"  from  the  other  one.  Thus  we  see  that  the  person  who  uses  the  reflex  A 
tends  to  provoke  others  to  obedience,  etc.  These  findings  involve  two-way  inter- 
personal phenomena  (what  the  subject  does  and  what  the  "Other"  does  back)  and  are 
therefore  less  reliable  than  the  other  interpersonal  codes  presented  in  this  figure.  The 
next  circle  illustrates  extreme  or  rigid  reflexes,  e.g.,  dominates.  The  perimeter  of  the 
circle  is  divided  into  eight  general  categories  employed  in  interpersonal  diagnosis. 
Fach  category  has  a  moderate  (adaptive)  and  an  extreme  (pathological)  intensity, 
e.g.,  Managerial-Autocratic. 


66  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

By  arranging  a  set  of  sixteen  interpersonal  variables  along  a  con- 
tinuum, we  have  implied  a  systematic  relationship  among  them.  If 
we  rate  any  behavior  as  C,  we  have  defined  it  in  terms  of  all  the  other 
variables  since  C  is  one  unit  away  from  (and  therefore  close  to)  D  and 
B,  while  it  is  eight  units  (and  therefore  quite  discrepant)  from  K.  The 
second  working  principle,  which  requires  a  relatedness  among  var- 
iables, is  thus  met  but  it  is  next  required  to  demonstrate  that  the  hypo- 
thetical relationships  of  these  variables  is  related  to  external  events. 
Extensive  vahdation  of  the  circular  continuum  of  sixteen  interpersonal 
variables  has  demonstrated  that  it  is  satisfactorily  congruent  with  em- 
pirical facts.  (5)  (8)  While  the  units  around  the  scale  are  not  com- 
pletely equidistant,  the  arrangement  is  correctly  ordered. 

The  selection  and  formal  organization  of  variables  made  it  possible 
to  rate  any  interpersonal  behavior  in  such  a  way  that  its  relationship  to 
all  the  other  fifteen  variables  was  explicit.  The  classificatory  schema 
at  this  stage  of  the  game  was  still  far  from  complete.  Only  the  most 
crude  appraisals  of  any  interpersonal  behavior  could  be  made  because 
only  the  presence  or  absence  of  the  theme  could  be  indicated.  For 
example,  it  was  possible  to  say  that  distrust  was  present;  but  how 
much,  how  extreme,  how  inappropriate  could  not  be  measured  until 
an  intensity  dimension  was  added. 

In  the  most  basic  sense  this  involved  making  a  "more  or  less  than" 
judgment  of  the  observed  event.  Is  this  behavior  more  distrustful  than 
the  other?  The  intensity  dimension  is  quite  fundamental  to  all  human 
perceptions.  Language  and  quantitative  usages  give  us  several  tech- 
niques for  expressing  intensities:  the  comparative  sequence  stroni!;, 
stronger,  strongest,  the  modifying  function  extremely,  slightly,  as  well 
as  the  intensity  hierarchy  of  different  word  meanings  critical-angry- 
furious-enraged.  Apart  from  these  verbal  expressions,  the  numerical 
estimation  of  intensity  (along  a  3-,  5-,  or  7-point  scale)  is  accepted  and 
common.  The  intensity  of  interpersonal  activity  can  be  rated  on  a 
linear  scale  ranging  from  absence  of  the  behavior  to  extreme  over- 
reactivity.  The  number  of  differentiating  points  on  the  intensity  scale 
can  vary  according  to  the  specific  purpose,  but  for  most  interpersonal 
responses,  a  3-  or  4-point  graduation  seems  quite  satisfactory. 

Let  us  consider,  by  way  of  illustration,  one  interpersonal  motivation 
as  it  is  reflected  in  the  intensity  dimension.  The  power  continuum 
(variable  A)  is  conceived  of  as  a  linear  scale  ranging  from  too  much  to 
complete  and  inappropriate  absence  of  dominance.  When  we  con- 
struct an  intensity  scale  for  each  of  the  sixteen  interpersonal  variables, 
we  obtain  a  more  differentiated  form  of  the  circular  continuum  which 
is  illustrated  in  the  concentric  rings  of  Figure  1.  The  term  dominate 
now  takes  on  quite  a  precise  meaning.  It  is  defined  as  an  expression  of 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  67 

power  (A)  which  systematically  relates  it  to  the  other  fifteen  inter- 
personal themes.  It  is  further  assigned  an  intensity  loading  which  re- 
lates it  to  all  other  verbal  terms  for  power  as  well  as  to  every  other 
classified  word  describing  interpersonal  interaction.  In  this  way 
language  of  personality  becomes  much  more  exact  and  accessible. 
Every  term  in  the  English  language  which  refers  to  interpersonal  be- 
havior can,  in  this  manner,  be  studied,  redefined  systematically,  and 
calibrated.  This  is  not  to  say  that  these  terms  as  used  in  everyday 
life  necessarily  have  the  same  meaning  to  the  interpersonal  scientist. 
The  general  public  employs  all  kinds  of  words — force,  power,  effi- 
ciency, hostility — which  have  been  operationally  redefined  by  physi- 
cal or  psychological  scientists.  The  interpersonal  diagnostician  dealing 
with  human  communications  has  to  keep  clear  the  level  of  meaning  of 
the  words  he  deals  with.  Anger  may  denote  one  thing  to  an  individual 
patient,  another  in  terms  of  general  usage,  and  a  third  in  the  precisely 
defined  scientific  discourse.  In  general,  it  seems  best  to  keep  the  scien- 
tific meaning  as  close  as  possible  to  that  of  the  general  public  of  the 
culture  being  studied.  The  advantages  of  tying  terminology  to  func- 
tional behavior  rather  than  tying  it  to  psychiatric  usage  have  already 
been  mentioned. 

The  two-dimensional  representation  of  interpersonal  space  has 
many  possibilities  for  summarizing  behavior.  First,  it  should  be  noted 
that  we  are  rarely  interested  in  classifying  single,  isolated  events.  In- 
variably we  are  concerned  with  sequences  of  interaction  and  patterns 
of  hundreds  of  interpersonal  expressions.  The  simplest  and  perhaps 
least  useful  way  of  summarizing  interpersonal  behavior  is  to  plot  the 
ratings,  judgments,  or  units  directly  onto  the  circle.  Suppose  we  re- 
cord and  then  rate  the  interpersonal  purpose  involved  in  everything 
a  patient  does  to  his  analyst  in  the  first  twenty  hours  of  therapy.  This 
would  produce  (depending  on  the  consistency  and  expressiveness  of 
the  patient)  between  1,000  and  3,000  interpersonal  units.  Disregard- 
ing the  intensity  ratings,  we  thus  obtain  the  total  of  all  Dominance  {A) 
ratings  and  the  comparable  totals  for  the  other  fifteen  interpersonal 
themes.  By  calibrating  the  sixteen  radii  for  numerical  frequency,  we 
can  then  strike  off  points  indicating  the  reactions  for  each  inter- 
personal variable.  A  graphic  summary  of  the  interpersonal  behavior 
during  twenty  hours  of  therapy  is  thus  obtained.  In  Figure  2  we  see 
that  the  sample  patient  manifested  docile,  cooperative  dependence 
toward  the  therapist,  avoiding  hostility  and  competitiveness.  Pro- 
files based  on  other  patients  or  upon  this  patient's  behavior  in  the  sub- 
sequent hours  of  treatment  would  allow  direct,  objective  comparisons 
and  the  testing  of  hypotheses  about  interpersonal  activity  during 
psychotherapy. 


68 


BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 


When  enough  cases  have  been  studied  to  provide  normative  data,  a 
second  and  highly  profitable  method  of  summarizing  interpersonal  be- 
havior is  possible.  There  are  many  statistical  techniques  for  treating 
each  patient's  scores  in  terms  of  the  mean  (i.e.,  the  average)  of  his 
group.   These  allow  us  to  determine  one  point  which  summarizes  all 


Figure  2.  Diagrammatic  Representation  of  Interpersonal  In- 
teraction of  a  Patient  During  Twenty  Hours  of  Psychotherapy. 
Radius  of  circle  equals  1,000  interactions.  This  patient  manifested 
820  docile-dependent  interpersonal  actions  {JK  octant)  and  260 
confident-narcissistic  actions  {BC  octant). 


of  the  interpersonal  behavior  in  any  behavioral  sequence  in  terms  of 
its  distance  and  direction  from  the  center  of  the  circle.  The  latter 
is  taken  as  the  mean,  i.e.,  the  central  tendency  of  the  interpersonal  be- 
havior of  the  population  studied.  One  method  for  obtaining  this 
summary  point  has  been  described  as  follows: 

The  Interpersonal  System  as  described  so  far  leaves  us  wide  latitude  with  respect 
to  the  formal  (algebraic)  properties  which  are  to  be  attributed  to  the  16 
variables.  We  may  in  fact  vary  the  formal  relationships  to  suit  the  particular 
context  so  long  as  we  do  not  violate  the  rough  intuitive  specification  of  a 
circular  arrangement.  For  example,  we  might  think  of  the  system  as  a  purely 
ordinal  array  about  which  one  specified  only  that  categories  adjacent  to  a  given 
one  resemble  it  more  than  do  non-adjacent  categories.  Or  we  might  consider 
the  circle  to  be  a  two-dimensional  array  in  ordinary  Euclidian  space,  in  which 
case  conventional  trigonometric  and  analytic  formulas  relate  the  16  variables. 
After  some  experimentation,  this  latter  approach  was  tentatively  selected.  Each 
circle  was  conceived  to  be  a  set  of  eight  vectors  or  points  in  a  two-dimensional 
space.  We  selected  the  center  of  gravit)^  or  vector  mean  of  these  points  as  a 
measure  of  central  tendency. 

A  vector  in  two-dimensional  space  may  be  represented  numerically  by  the 
magnitude  of  its  components  in  two  arbitrarily  selected  directions.  We  chose 
AP  and  LAI  as  reference  directions,  giving  the  designations  Dom  and  Lov 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTE/MS  69 

respectively  to  the  components  of  the  vector  sum  in  these  two  directions. 
Representation  of  the  eight  or  sixteen  scores  comprising  a  patient's  circle  by  a 
single  point  in  two-dimensional  space  is  a  considerable  simplification.  What  is 
preserved  in  this  simplification  is  the  general  tendency  of  the  circle.  What  is 
lost  are  the  individual  fluctuations  around  the  circle.^ 

The  formulas  for  the  two  components  of  the  vector  sum  are  relatively 
evident.  They  are: 

1.  Dom  =  i«Ri  sin  ^,  and 

t 
\=  1 

2.  Lov   =  i«R,  cos  0i 

2 
i=  1 

where  R,  =  the  score  in  the  1-th  category, 

01  =  the  angle  made  by  moving  in  counter-clockwise  direction  from  L 
to  the  i-th  category  (from  LM  if  octant  scores  are  used). 

In  the  present  calculations,  octant  scores  were  used  and  .7  was  taken  as  the 
value  of  sin  45°;  the  following  simplified  formulas  resulted: 

3.  Dom  =AP  -HI  +.7  (NO  +  BC  -  FG  -  JK), 

4.  Lov    =LM-DE  +  .7  (NO  -  BC  -  FG  +  JK), 
where  AP  =  score  in  octant  AP,  etc.   (4,  p.  140) 

It  is  thus  possible  to  convert  the  pattern  of  scores  on  the  sixteen 
variables  into  two  numerical  indices  which  locate  a  subject's  inter- 
personal behavior  on  a  diagnostic  grid.  Figure  3  presents  the  descrip- 
tive summary  point  for  the  therapy  patient  whose  behavior  has  been 
previously  diagramed  in  Figure  2.  We  note  that  the  two  summary 
indices  place  him  in  the  JK  octant;  they  thus  become  a  simplified  and 
numerical  summary  of  the  circular  diagram.  The  vertical  and  hori- 
zontal lines  represent  varying  discrepancies  from  the  mean  (the  center 
point  of  the  circle).  We  obtain  in  this  manner  a  circular  grid,  every 
point  on  which  is  statistically  defined.  We  determine  the  summary 
point  of  the  patient's  interpersonal  behavior  as  rated  by  the  sixteen 
variables  in  relationship  to  the  population  studied — which  in  this  case 
might  be  a  hundred  randomly  selected  psychotherapy  patients.  Our 
subject  is  seen  as  considerably  more  trustful  and  compliant  than  the 
average  therapy  patient  (point  1  in  Figure  3). 

The  great  advantage  of  the  latter  circular  grid  method  of  summari- 
zation is  that  many  summary  points  can  be  graphed  on  the  same  dia- 

'  The  two  components  of  the  vector  sum  must  each  be  divided  by  N  =  Ri   (the 

i=  1 
total  around  the  circle  all  eight  or  sixteen  scores)  to  get  the  two  components  of  the 
vector  mean.  These  latter  may  also  be  thought  of  as  the  first  two  Fourier  coefficients 
of  a  curve  fitted  to  the  observed  data.   More  complicated  curves  can  be  fitted  by  the 
computation  of  additional  coefficients. 


70 


BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 


gram,  facilitating  comparison  among  levels  of  any  individual's  per- 
sonality or  comparisons  among  different  individuals.  Let  us  suppose 
that  the  psychotherapy  patient  we  have  been  using  for  illustration 
shifted  his  interpersonal  behavior  markedly  in  the  second  twenty  hours 


:.^^^S0CH1ST\C 

^(Hjr ' 

Figure  3.  Diagram  Summarizing  the  Interpersonal  Behavior  of 
Patient  During  First  Twenty  Hours  (T)  and  the  Second  Twenty 
Hours  ®  of  Psychotherapy.  Summary  points  are  located  by  inter- 
section of  horizontal  and  vertical  indices.  The  indices  are  deter- 
mined by  the  raw  number  of  interactions  converted  to  vector 
scores  by  the  trigonometric  formulas  described  on  page  69. 

of  treatment,  expressing  disappointment  and  distrust  towards  the 
analyst.  The  several  thousand  interactions  are  rated,  statistically  sum- 
marized, and  graphed  as  point  2^on  Figure  3.  A  diagrammatic  con- 
densation of  the  changing  behavior  of  the  patient  (based  on  quanti- 
tative objective  methods)  becomes  available.  This  patient  has  shifted 
his  interpersonal  behavior  in  therapy.    He  was  compliant  (point  1) 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS 


71 


during  the  initial  stage  of  treatment  but  became  passively  hostile  and 
withdrawn  in  the  second  stage  of  therapy.  Later  changes  in  the  treat- 
ment relationship  can  be  similarly  plotted — always  in  relationship  to 
the  average  of  the  population. 

Previous  Suggestions  for  a  Two-Dimensional 
Classification  of  Personality  Traits 

The  notion  of  classifying  human  emotions  in  terms  of  four  syste- 
matically related  variables  is  certainly  not  novel.  The  history  of 
psychology  provides  several  interesting  correspondences  to  the  present 
system  of  arranging  data  in  terms  of  the  four  nodal  points. 

The  four  quadrants  of  the  interpersonal  system  comprise  blends  of 
the  nodal  dichotomies:  love  versus  hate  and  power  versus  weakness. 
The  four  "blended"  quadrants  fit  rather  closely  the  classical  humors 
theory  of  Hippocrates.  The  upper  left  quadrant  (hostile  strength) 
equates  with  the  choleric  temperament,  the  lower  left  (hostile  weak- 
ness) with  the  melancholic,  the  lower  right  (friendly  weakness)  with 
the  phlegmatic,  and  the  upper  right  (friendly  strength)  with  the 
sanguine. 

The  same  fourfold  classification  reappears  in  Freudian  thought, 
Freud's  treatment  of  the  individual  stresses  two  basic  motives — love 
and  hate.  His  theories  of  social  phenomena  and  group  interaction,  on 
the  other  hand,  emphasize  domination,  power,  and  the  interaction  of 
the  weak  versus  the  strong.  In  his  open  letter  to  Einstein  "Why  War?" 
these  two  avenues  of  Freud's  thought  intersect  and  illustrate  his 
commitment  to  the  four  concepts.  He  presents  his  power  theory  first: 

Such  then,  was  the  original  state  of  things:  domination  by  whoever  had  the 
greater  might— domination  by  hate  violence  or  by  violence  supported  by 
intellect.    (2,  p.  275) 

In  the  following  paragraph  he  says: 

The  situation  is  simple  so  long  as  the  community  consists  only  of  a  number 
of  equally  strong  individuals.  .  .  .  But  a  state  of  rest  of  that  kind  is  only  theo- 
retically conceivable.  In  actuality,  the  position  is  complicated  by  the  fact  that 
from  its  very  beginning  the  community  comprises  elements  of  unequal  strength 
—men  and  women,  parents  and  children— and  soon,  as  a  result  of  war  and  con- 
quest, it  also  comes  to  include  victors  and  vanquished,  who  turn  into  masters 
and  slaves.  The  justice  of  the  community  then  becomes  an  expression  of  the 
unequal  degrees  of  power  obtaining  within  it;  the  laws  are  made  by  and  for  the 
ruling  members  and  find  little  room  for  the  rights  of  those  in  subjection.  From 
that  time  forward  there  are  two  factors  at  work  in  the  community  which  are 
sources  of  unrest  over  matters  of  law  but  tend  at  the  same  time  to  a  further 
growth  of  law.  First,  attempts  are  made  by  certain  of  the  rulers  to  set  them- 
selves above  the  prohibitions  which  apply  to  everyone— they  seek,  that  is,  to  go 


y2  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

back  from  a  dominion  of  law  to  a  dominion  of  violence.  Secondly,  the  op- 
pressed members  of  the  group  make  constant  efforts  to  obtain  more  power  and 
to  have  any  constant  efforts  to  obtain  more  power  and  to  have  any  changes  that 
are  brought  about  in  that  direction  recognized  in  the  laws— they  press  forward, 
that  is,  from  unequal  justice  to  equal  justice  for  all.   (2,  pp.  276-77) 

Later,  in  the  same  paper,  Freud  goes  on  to  summarize  his  familiar 
theories  of  individual  motivation. 

According  to  our  hypothesis  human  instincts  are  of  only  two  kinds:  those 
which  seek  to  preserve  and  unite— which  we  call  "erotic,"  exactly  in  the  sense 
in  which  Plato  used  the  word  "Eros"  in  his  Symposium,  or  "sexual"  with  a 
deliberate  extension  of  the  popular  conception  of  "sexuality"— and  those  which 
seek  to  destroy  and  kill  and  which  we  class  together  as  the  aggressive  or  destruc- 
tive instinct.  As  you  see,  this  is  in  fact  no  more  than  a  theoretical  clarification 
of  the  universally  familiar  opposition  between  Love  and  Hate  which  may  per- 
haps have  some  fundamental  relation  to  the  polarity  of  attraction  and  repulsion 
that  plays  a  part  in  your  own  field  of  knowledge.  We  must  not  be  too  hasty 
in  introducing  ethical  judgments  of  good  and  evil.  Neither  of  these  instincts  is 
any  less  essential  than  the  other,  the  phenomena  of  life  arise  from  the  operation 
of  both  together,  whether  acting  in  concert  or  in  opposition.  It  seems  as 
though  an  instinct  of  the  one  sort  can  scarcely  ever  operate  in  isolation;  it  is 
always  accompanied— or,  as  we  say,  alloyed— with  an  element  from  the  other 
side,  which  modifies  its  aim  or  is,  in  some  cases,  what  enables  it  to  achieve  that 
aim.  Thus,  for  instance,  the  instinct  of  self-preservation  is  certainly  of  an 
erotic  kind,  but  it  must  nevertheless  have  aggressiveness  at  its  disposal  if  it  is  to 
fulfill  its  purpose.  So,  too,  the  instinct  of  love,  when  it  is  directed  toward  an 
object,  stands  in  need  of  some  contribution  from  the  instinct  of  mastery  if  it  is 
in  any  way  to  possess  that  object.  The  difficulty  of  isolating  the  two  classes  of 
instinct  in  their  actual  manifestations  is  indeed  what  has  so  long  prevented  us 
from  recognizing  them. 

If  you  will  follow  me  a  little  further,  you  will  see  that  human  actions  are 
subject  to  another  complication  of  a  different  kind.  It  is  very  rarely  that  an 
action  is  the  work  of  a  smgle  instinctual  impulse  (which  must  in  itself  be  com- 
pounded of  Eros  and  destructiveness).  In  order  to  make  an  action  possible, 
there  must  be  as  a  rule  a  combination  of  such  compounded  motives.  This  was 
perceived  long  ago  by  a  specialist  in  your  own  subject,  a  Professor  G.  C. 
Lichtenberg  who  taught  physics  at  Gottingen  during  our  classical  age-though 
perhaps  he  was  even  more  remarkable  as  a  psychologist  than  as  a  physicist.  He 
invented  a  Compass  of  Motives,  for  he  wrote.  "The  motives  that  lead  us  to  do 
anything  might  be  arranged  like  the  thirty-two  winds  and  might  be  given 
names  on  the  same  pattern:  for  instance,  'food-food-fame'  or  'fame-fame- 
food'.  So  that  when  human  beings  are  incited  to  war  they  may  have  a  whole 
number  of  motives  for  assenting— some  noble  and  some  base,  some  of  which 
they  speak  openly  and  others  on  which  thev  are  silent.  There  is  no  need  to 
enumerate  them  all.  A  lust  for  aggression  and  destruction  is  certainly  among 
them:  the  countless  cruelties  in  history  and  in  our  every  day  lives  vouch  for  its 
existence  and  its  strength.  The  gratification  of  these  destructive  impulses  is  of 
course  facilitated  bv  their  admixture  with  others  of  an  erotic  and  idealistic 
kind."   (2,  pp.  280-82) 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS 


73 


The  similarity  between  these  suggestions  for  a  "Compass  of  Mo- 
tives" and  the  circular  classificatory  system  described  in  this  book  is 
so  close  as  to  require  no  further  comment. 

In  addition  to  these  earlier  approaches  to  a  fourfold  classification 
system  of  human  motives,  other  similar  conceptual  schemes  have  been 
developed  contemporaneously  with  (and  independently  of)  the  inter- 
personal system. 

The  interpersonal  system,  it  will  be  recalled,  was  developed  from 
the  rawest  kind  of  empirical  approach.  It  can  be  said  that  the  patients 
in  the  earliest  pilot  study  group  developed  the  interpersonal  circle  by 
providing  the  varied  pool  of  interpersonal  responses  which  were 
gradually  refined  into  the  present  circular  continuum.  It  is  most  inter- 
esting, therefore,  that  the  results  of  our  empirical  studies  tend  to  con- 
firm hypotheticated  fourfold  classifications  independently  proposed 
by  other  writers. 

Ross  Stagner,  for  example,  in  1937  presented  a  two-dimensional 
representation  of  behavior  which  has  a  certain  similarity  to  the  inter- 
personal "compass."  Stagner  wrote:  "The  hypothesis  which  we  wish 
to  present  is  that  the  directions  of  variability  in  human  behavior  are 
very  limited  in  number,  present  evidence  suggesting  that  there  are 
only  two  dimensions  along  which  such  variations  may  be  plotted. 
These  two  dimensions  may  be  considered:  1)  approach  to  or  with- 
drawal from  a  stimulus  object;  and  2)  increased  or  decreased  organis- 
mic  activity  with  reference  to  the  object."  (II,  p.  52) 

Although  Stagner  is  noninterpersonal  in  his  variable  system  and, 
perhaps,  overly  optimistic  about  the  simplicity  of  direction  and  moti- 
vation, his  paradigm  attracts  our  interest  for  two  reasons:  First,  it  is 
remarkably  similar  to  the  interpersonal  circular  system.  Secondly,  it 
is  close  to  the  spatial  theory  of  the  genesis  of  interpersonal  relations 
which  we  have  discussed  in  the  preceding  pages. 

Talcott  Parsons,  who  is  perhaps  the  most  sophisticated  and  syste- 
matically mature  sociological  writer  of  our  generation,  has  described 
a  conceptual  method  which  he  calls  the  "paradigm  of  motivational 
process."  He  states  that  this 

.  .  .  started  with  the  assumption  that  a  process  of  interaction  which  has  been 
stabilized  about  conformity  with  a  normative  pattern  structure,  will  tend  to 
continue  in  a  stable  state  unless  it  is  disturbed.  Concretely,  however,  there  will 
always  be  tendencies  to  deviance,  and  conversely  these  tendencies  will  tend  to 
be  counteracted  by  re-equilibrating  processes,  on  the  part  of  the  same  actor  or 
of  others. 

It  was  furthermore  maintained  that  neither  the  tendencies  toward  deviance 
nor  those  toward  re-equilibration,  that  is,  toward  "social  control"  could  occur 
in  random  directions  or  forms.    Deviance  was  shown  to  involve  four  basic 


74  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

directions,  according  to  whether  the  need  was  to  express  alienation  from  the 
normative  pattern— including  the  repudiation  of  attachment  to  alter  as  an  object 
—or  to  maintain  compulsive  conformity  with  the  normative  pattern  and  attach- 
ment to  alter,  and  according  to  whether  the  mode  of  action  was  actively  or 
passively  inclined.  This  yielded  four  directional  types,  those  of  aggressiveness 
and  withdrawal  on  the  alienative  side,  and  of  compulsive  performance  and 
compulsive  acceptance  on  the  side  of  compulsive  conformity.  It  was  further- 
more shown  that  this  paradigm,  independently  derived,  is  essentially  the 
same  as  that  previously  put  forward  by  Merton  for  the  analysis  of  social  struc- 
ture and  anomie.  (9,  p.  68) 

Thus,  we  see  that  two  productive  sociologists,  Parsons  and  Merton, 
although  working  from  somewhat  different  subject  matters  and  frames 
of  reference,  have  arrived  at  solutions  for  categorizing  human  inter- 
action which  are  close  to  the  interpersonal  circle. 

Another  very  interesting  correspondence  has  developed  from  the 
researches  of  George  T.  Lodge.  Lodge  has  developed  some  promising 
applications  of  the  Haskell  Coaction  technique  to  psychological  meas- 
urement. This  is  a  method  for  plotting  the  resolution  of  two  coacting 
variables  in  terms  of  a  two-dimensional  surface.  The  coaction  compass 
functions  exactly  as  the  interpersonal  circle,  and  the  standard  trigono- 
metric solutions  of  coaction  variables  have  been  applied  by  Haskell  and 
Lodge  to  their  data  in  the  same  manner  as  La  Forge's  formulas  for 
the  interpersonal  system. 

Lodge  describes  his  use  of  the  Haskell  Coaction  Compass  method 
as  follows: 

The  Coaction  Compass  as  formulated  by  Edward  F.  Haskell  is  a  general 
conceptual  scheme  which  is  beginning  to  find  wide  applications  in  biological 
and  social  science.  This  compass  is  a  Cartesian  coordinate  frame  strictly  com- 
parable to  the  mariner's  wind  rose.  Its  use  permits  assignment  of  vector  magni- 
tudes to  the  resultant  forces  from  any  two  interdependent  power  systems,  and 
their  subsequent  treatment  by  methods  of  analytic  geometry.  In  the  field  of 
Clinical  Psychology,  it  is  convenient  to  view  the  processes  of  inhibition  and 
facilitation  as  representing  two  such  coacting  power  systems.  It  is  not  our 
purpose  at  present  to  go  into  the  details  of  a  coaction  theory  of  personality  as 
such.  We  have  attempted  a  preliminary  formulation  of  such  a  theory  else- 
where. Here,  we  shall  try  only  to  set  forth  certain  necessary  steps  for  the 
interest  of  those  who  may  wish  to  apply  coaction  reasoning  in  their  analyses 
of  Rorschach  protocols.  .  .  . 

The  Rorschach  method  lends  itself  readily  to  the  study  of  personality  in 
terms  of  a  coaction  formulation,  at  least  insofar  as  consideration  of  the  scoring 
of  determinants  is  concerned.  If  the  form  level  of  a  response  be  regarded  as 
reflecting  the  strength  of  the  inhibitory  process,  and  if  the  amount  of  expression 
of  color,  shading,  and  movement  be  regarded  as  reflecting  the  level  of  manifest 
affect  or  facilitation,  the  response  may  be  represented  geometrically  as  a  re- 
sultant vector  determined  by  the  relative  strengths  of  the  two  coacting  power 
systems.    (6,  pp.  67h58) 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  75 

The  Variability  of  Interpersonal  Behavior 

Eitiploying  the  continuum  of  sixteen  variables,  summarized  nu- 
merically, it  is  possible  to  make  three  different  types  of  systematic 
studies  of  the  same  person.  We  can  investigate  the  interpersonal  be- 
havior of  one  individual  at  many  levels  of  his  personality.  Charting 
the  measurements  for  all  aspects  of  behavior  on  the  same  circular  grid 
provides  a  systematic  pattern  diagnosis  of  the  structure  of  personality 
at  one  time.  By  adding  summaries  of  the  same  measurements  as  they 
change  in  time,  we  obtain  a  picture  of  temporal  variation  in  the 
multilevel  pattern  of  personality.  In  the  preceding  example  we  have 
noted  such  a  temporal  change  in  one  level  of  personality — inter- 
personal behavior  in  one  cultural  context,  the  psychoanalytic  ses- 
sions. A  third  use  of  the  circular  continuum  is  to  chart  the  varying 
patterns  of  behavior  in  different  interpersonal  situations.  How  does 
the  patient  behave  with  his  boss,  with  his  wife,  with  his  children? 

These  measurements  of  behavior,  at  different  levels,  at  different 
times,  and  in  different  situations  comprise  the  basic  patterns  and 
changing  processes  of  personality.  They  are  called  structural,  tem- 
poral, and  situational  variation  patterns,  respectively.  Temporal  varia- 
tion— the  changes  in  personality  patterns  over  time — has  extreme 
functional  importance  since  our  prediction  about  future  developments 
(e.g.,  prognosis  for  psychotherapy)  is  involved.  Situational  variation 
refers  to  the  cultural  relativity  of  interpersonal  relationships.  Struc- 
tural variation  refers  to  the  relationship  among  the  levels  of  personal- 
ity and  brings  us  to  the  basic  issues  of  the  notational  system — the  or- 
ganization of  personahty  into  levels. 

The  Formal  Notational  System:  The  Levels  of  Personality 

The  fact  that  behavior  exists  at  more  than  one  level  of  awareness 
has  been  intuitively  recognized  for  centuries.  The  discovery  of  un- 
conscious motivation — in  the  sense  of  a  formal  theoretical  statement — 
was  first  made  by  Sigmund  Freud. 

This  was  an  epochal  landmark  in  the  study  of  personality  and 
human  nature. 

The  neat  personality  structures  of  rationalistic  psychology  were 
exploded  into  an  untidy  disarray.  It  is  no  longer  possible  to  depend 
on  the  solid  validity  of  the  subject's  conscious  report.  If  the  subject 
in  a  perception  experiment  judges  one  stimulus  object  as  larger  than 
another,  it  may  have  to  do  with  the  physical  aspects  of  perception — 
but  it  may  also  reflect  a  desire  to  agree  or  disagree  with  other  subjects, 
to  assist  or  frustrate  the  experimenter's  purpose  (as  he  imagines  it  to 
be). 


76  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

The  concept  of  levels  destroys  the  simple,  unidimensional  notions  of 
behavior  determined  by  chains  of  stimulus-response  reactions.  All  the 
major  learning  theories  since  Freud,  however  cognitive  and  physical- 
istic  they  may  strive  to  be,  have  by  necessity  taken  into  account  this 
multidimensional  quality  of  motivation.  The  complexity  of  human  na- 
ture for  the  first  time  begins  to  command  adequate  conceptual  respect. 

Accompanying  the  early  positive  rewards  of  the  "unconsciousness 
theory"  is  a  series  of  premature,  intuitive  concepts  and  logical  falla- 
cies. To  deal  with  some  of  these  illogical  procedures,  we  have  stated 
in  the  fifth  working  principle  that  any  statement  about  human  be- 
havior must  indicate  the  level  of  personality  data  to  which  it  refers. 

When  this  postulate  was  applied  to  the  varied  mosaic  of  miscel- 
laneous protocols  obtained  from  the  pilot  study  cases,  the  first  task 
required  was  to  classify  them  into  discrete  levels.  The  questions  then 
became:  How  many  levels  of  personality  should  be  employed?  What 
are  they?    And  how  shall  they  be  defined? 

Any  solutions  to  these  problems  must  be  arbitrary,  formal  decisions. 
That  is,  we  must  assume  no  divinely  instituted  or  platonically  ideal 
number  of  personality  divisions.  In  selecting  the  number  of  levels, 
we  are  limited  on  the  broad  side  by  the  practicalities  of  the  empirical 
method  and  on  the  narrow  side  by  theoretical  adequacy,  that  is  (at 
this  primitive  state  of  our  knowledge),  if  we  have  too  many  levels,  the 
permutations  and  combinations  of  the  interlevel  relationships  become 
impossibly  unwieldy.  If  we  have  too  few,  important  nuances  become 
lost  by  being  compressed  into  general  categories. 

After  reviewing  the  many  types  and  sources  of  personality  data, 
a  classification  into  five  levels  was  found  to  be  the  most  effective.  This 
decision  is  a  notational  procedure  which  seems  to  meet  the  functional 
criteria  of  the  present  time.  When  we  say  that  it  is  convenient  to  con- 
ceive of  five  levels  of  personality,  we  do  not  imply  that  there  is  "really" 
or  "eternally"  such  a  structural  division.  Early  psychoanalytic  writers 
naively  tended  to  imply,  and  the  uncritical  reader  tended  to  assume, 
that  there  "really  were"  two  or  three  levels  of  personality  in  the  same 
sense  that  there  "are"  five  fingers  on  the  hand.  When  the  formal  na- 
ture of  these  divisions  of  consciousness  was  not  made  explicit,  a  meta- 
physical language  threatened  to  develop.  At  this  point  we  designate 
five  levels  of  personality  data  which  we  suggest  are  the  most  profitable 
for  research,  theory,  and  functional  prediction. 

These  five  general  levels  of  personality  data  are:  I.  the  Level  of 
Public  Communication;  II.  the  Level  of  Conscious  Description;  III. 
the  Level  of  Private  Symbolization;  IV.  the  Level  of  the  Unexpressed 
Unconscious;  and  V,  the  Level  of  Values.  These  levels  are  defined  in 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  77 

terms  of  the  operations  which  produce  the  pertinent  data.  That  is, 
the  source  of  the  data  automatically  determines  the  level  of  classifica- 
tion. In  this  way  we  obtain  operational  definitions  of  the  five  levels 
of  personality. 

There  are  many  different  specific  kinds  of  expression  which  can 
contribute  data  to  any  one  level.  For  example,  there  are  several  ways 
in  which  fantasy  symbols  can  be  manifested — dreams,  projective  tests, 
fantasies,  etc.  All  of  these  produce  Level  III  data,  although  the  opera- 
tions by  which  the  themes  are  expressed  are  quite  separate.  In  order 
to  insure  clarity  and  precision  we  always  indicate  (by  code)  the  spe- 
cific source  of  the  data.  The  general  level  is  designated  by  a  roman 
numeral  and  the  sublevel  operations  are  designated  by  a  code  letter. 
Level  III-D,  for  example,  means  private  interpersonal  symbols  ob- 
tained from  dreams.  Level  III-T  indicates  private  interpersonal  sym- 
bols obtained  from  TAT  stories.  The  general  definition  of  levels  and 
the  specific  test  and  rating  procedures  by  which  they  are  measured 
will  now  be  presented. 

Level  I  (Public  Communication)  consists  of  the  overt  behavior  of 
the  individual  as  rated  by  others  along  the  sixteen-point  circular  con- 
tinuum. These  judgments  are  made  by  trained  observers  or  by  naive 
fellow  subjects  who  observe  the  subject  in  interpersonal  situations. 
They  rate  his  interpersonal  impact  as  it  appears  to  them.  What  we  ob- 
tain is  a  series  of  ratings  of  the  interpersonal  effect  the  subject  has  on 
others  who  share  social  situations  with  him.  Other  estimates  of  Level 
I  behavior  are  obtained  from  special  test  procedures — situation  test, 
prediction  scales  and  the  like. 

Level  I  data  is  objective  or  public — rather  than  private  or  subjec- 
tive. It  may  or  may  not  agree  with  the  subject's  own  view  of  the  situa- 
tion. To  obtain  Level  I  data  it  is  necessary  to  have  the  subject  in- 
volved in  social  interaction  and  to  have  others  rate  their  view  of  his 
purposive  behavior.  This  gives  a  measurement  of  his  social  "stimulus 
value."  Other  specialized  methods  for  assessing  Level  I  require  the 
patient  to  take  criterion-specific  tests  (like  the  MMPI)  which  allow 
us  to  predict  his  interpersonal  role. 

The  situation  in  which  we  rate  interpersonal  behavior  can  be  an 
extraclinic  event  or  it  can  be  restricted  to  the  more  controlled  en- 
vironment of  the  clinic  or  assessment  situation.  The  raters  can  be  re- 
searchers, diagnostic  or  therapeutic  clinicians,  fellow  patients,  or 
family  members.  The  meaning  of  the  Level  I  rating  thus  depends  on 
the  cultural  context  and  the  category  of  the  rater.  These  differences 
provide  interesting  sublevel  variations  of  the  broad,  general  Level  I 
of  Public  Communication. 


78 


BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 


There  are  five  methods  which  provide  estimates  of  Level  I  public 
behavior.  These  are  coded  as  follows: 

Level  I-Al:  MMPI  indices  which  reflect  the  interpersonal  pressure 
exerted  on  the  clinician  by  the  patient's  symptoms. 

Level  I-R:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  patient's  minute-by- 
minute  behavior  in  a  social  situation. 

Level  I-S:  Sociometric  ratings  (from  check  lists)  by  fellow  patients 
or  by  trained  observers. 

Level  I-P:  MMPI  indices  wliich  predict  the  interpersonal  behavior 
to  be  expected  in  group  psychotherapy. 

Level  I-T:  Scores  from  standard  situational  tests  which  assess  the 
patient's  interpersonal  reactions. 

The  following  chapter  is  devoted  to  a  detailed  description  of  the  im- 
phcations,  measurement,  and  validation  of  Level  I  behavior. 

Level  II  {Conscious  Descriptions)  includes  the  verbal  content  of 
all  the  statements  that  the  subject  makes  about  the  interpersonal  be- 
havior of  himself  or  "others."  His  descriptions  of  himself  and  others 
are  obtained  from  a  variety  of  sources — conversations,  therapy  proto- 
cols, autobiographies,  check  lists.  They  are  then  rated  along  the  same 
sixteen-point  circular  continuum.  We  are  interested  here  in  the  sub- 
ject's reported  perceptions  of  himself  and  his  interpersonal  world.  We 
are  not  interested  at  this  level  in  the  consensual  accuracy  of  these  per- 
ceptions or  in  the  potential  deeper  meanings.  We  are  concerned  only 
with  the  phenomenological  field — the  way  in  which  the  subject  re- 
ports his  view  of  self  and  world.  It  must  be  noted  that  one  single  sen- 
tence expressed  by  a  subject  can  provide  both  a  Level  I  and  a  Level  II 
rating.  If  a  patient  says,  "I  am  a  responsible  person,"  the  Level  II  rat- 
ing reflects  the  surface  meaning  of  responsibility  (coded  as  O)  re- 
ported by  the  subject.  Observers  of  the  interpersonal  context  in  which 
the  sentence  was  uttered  might  agree  that  its  Level  I-R  effect  was  to 
establish  autonomy  from  the  therapist  (coded  B)  or  superiority  over 
other  patients  (also  coded  B).  The  reported  self-perception  usually  is 
different  from  the  interpersonal  impact  on  or  meaning  to  others. 

There  are  four  methods  which  provide  data  for  Level  II  descrip- 
tions of  self  and  others.  These  are  coded  as  follows: 

Level  II-Di:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  verbal  content  from 
diagnostic  interviews. 

Level  II-Ti:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  verbal  content  from 
therapy  interviews. 

Level  II-C:,  Scores  from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  on 
which  the  patient  checks  his  view  of  self  and  others. 

Level  II-A:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  content  of  autobiog- 
raphies written  by  patients. 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS 


79 


The  illustration,  implications,  use,  and  validation  of  this  level  of  con- 
scious description  will  be  considered  in  Chapter  8. 

Level  III  {Private  Symbolization)  consists  of  projective,  indirect 
fantasy  materials.  These  data  come  from  a  variety  of  sources — dreams, 
fantasies,  artistic,  or  autistic  productions,  projective  tests — which 
elicit  imaginative  expressions.  The  interpersonal  themes  of  all  these 
symbolic  expressions  are  rated  by  two  or  more  trained  raters  along 
the  sixteen-point  circular  continuum.  We  thus  possess  a  technique 
for  systematically  measuring  the  indirect  autistic  data  of  personality  in 
terms  of  the  same  interpersonal  variables  which  we  use  to  categorize 
the  public  or  conscious  aspects  of  behavior.  The  broad  general  nature 
of  the  level  categories  must  be  mentioned  again.  There  are  many  sub- 
level  varieties  of  symbolic  data.  Some  creative,  projective  tests,  for 
example,  may  be  more  closely  related  to  the  level  of  conscious  de- 
scription. Others  may  be  consistently  identified  with  the  pattern  of 
dream  themes.  The  exact  "depth"  of  any  symbolic  response  depends 
on  a  variety  of  factors — cultural  context,  type  of  symbolic  stimulus, 
the  nature  of  the  Level  I  behavior  at  the  time,  etc.  The  detailed  sys- 
tematic organization  and  specific  differentiation  of  these  private  pro- 
ductions becomes  one  of  the  most  important  and  fascinating  problems 
of  current  dynamic  psychology. 

There  are  at  present  seven  methods  for  collecting  Level  III  pre- 
conscious  data  from  patients.  These  are  coded  as  follows: 

Level  III-T:       Ratings  of  TAT  stories. 

Level  III-IFT:  Ratings  from  the  Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test.^ 
Level  Ill-i:         Ratings  of  responses  to  the  Iflund  projective  test.   (3) 
Level  III-B:       Ratings  of  responses  the  Blacky  projective  test.  (1) 
Level  III-D:       Ratings  of  interpersonal  themes  in  dream  protocols. 
Level  III-F:       Ratings  of  interpersonal  themes  from  waking  fan- 
tasies expressed  by  the  subject. 
Level  III-M:      MMPI   indices   which  predict  to   preconscious  be- 
havior. 

There  is  one  distinction  to  be  made  in  dealing  with  preconscious 
data  that  is  most  important.  This  is  the  division  between  the  hero  and 
the  world  personages  in  fantasy  productions.  Evidence  from  several 
samples  suggests  that  clearly  different  sublevels  of  behavior  are  in- 
volved. 

*  The  Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test  is  a  Level  III  instrument  developed  by  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  psychology  research  project  to  fit  the  interpersonal  system.  It  is  a  TAT- 
type  test  in  which  the  cards  are  designed  to  explore  systematically  the  subject's  fan- 
tasies about  interpersonal  relationships  between  heroes  and  paternal,  maternal,  cross- 
sex,  and  same-sex  figures.  Scores  are  obtained  for  Level  III  Self,  Mother,  Father, 
Cross-sex  and  Therapist. 


8o  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

These  findings  are  of  considerable  value  because  they  define  two 
distinct  sublevels  of  the  symbolic  or  preconscious  area.  One  is  desig- 
nated Level  III  Hero.  This  is  the  symbolic  self-image.  Its  theoretical 
and  clinical  meaning  is  different  from  the  preconscious  images  of  the 
symboKc  world.  This  latter  area  is  designated  Level  III  Other.  These 
two  subdivisions  of  symbolic  expression  have  been  found  to  be  lawfully 
distinct.  They  often  define  different  kinds  of  interlevel  conflict  and 
different  personality  types,  and  they  are  related  to  different  sympto- 
matic pictures.  Chapter  9  which  is  devoted  to  Level  III  symbolic  be- 
havior will  consider  these  distinctions. 

Level  IV  (the  Unexpressed  Unconscious)  is  defined  by  the  inter- 
personal themes  which  are  systematically  and  compulsively  avoided 
by  the  subject  at  all  the  other  levels  of  personality  and  which  are 
conspicuous  by  their  inflexible  absence.  Here  we  refer  to  those  activi- 
ties which  are  consistently  and  deliberately  "not  present"  in  the  per- 
sonality profile.  These  "unexpressed"  aspects  of  personality  are  as  yet 
unexplored.  For  this  reason,  this  level  will  not  be  employed  in  the 
basic  systematization  that  follows. 

The  definition  of  Level  IV  is  a  problem  as  yet  unsolved.  The  most 
convincing  demonstration  of  the  presence  of  motivation  previously 
unexpressed  (at  the  other  three  levels)  would  require  two  parallel  sets 
of  evidence.  The  negative  proof  would  involve  statistical  demonstra- 
tion that  the  subject  significantly  avoids  certain  patterns  of  interper- 
sonal response  with  a  frequency  far  beyond  the  expectations  of 
chance.  The  proof  positive  requires  that  the  same  interpersonal  themes 
be  picked  up  in  significant  frequency  by  certain  subliminal,  indirect 
perceptual  tests,  e.g.,  abnormally  long  reaction  times  or  perceptual 
distortions  in  response  to  thematic  stimuli  presented  at  spht-second 
(blurred)  tachistoscope  exposures.  The  implications  and  problems  in- 
volved in  the  unexpressed  behavior  of  Level  IV  will  be  surveyed  in 
Chapter  10. 

Level  V  (Values)  consists  of  the  data  which  reflect  the  subject's 
system  of  moral,  "superego  judgments,"  his  ego  ideal.  We  refer  here 
to  the  interpersonal  traits  and  actions  that  the  subject  holds  to  be 
"good,"  proper,  and  "right" — his  picture  of  how  he  should  be  and 
would  like  to  be.  These  idealized  interpersonal  themes  are  obtained 
in  the  same  manner  as  the  conscious  descriptions  of  Level  II.  We  single 
out  from  interview,  free  association,  check  list,  and  questionnaire  the 
expressions  which  concern  his  value-feelings.  These  are  rated  and 
scored  according  to  the  sixteen-point  circular  continuum. 

Like  the  other  levels  of  personality,  the  "ego  ideal"  cannot  be  con- 
ceived of  as  a  unitary  or  narrowly  defined  category.  Some  "values" 
may  be  consciously  expressed — others  may  be  rated  as  they  appear 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  8i 

in  implied  form.  Thus  some  may  be  "deeper"  than  others.  There  are 
three  methods  for  obtaining  Level  V  ratings  of  the  ego  ideal.  These  are 
coded  as  follows: 

Level  V-C:  Scores  from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  on 
which  the  patient  checks  his  ego  ideal. 

Level  V-Di:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  subject's  ideals  as 
expressed  in  diagnostic  interviews. 

Level  V-Ti:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  subject's  ideals  as 
expressed  in  therapy  interviews. 

The  measurement  and  meaning  of  this  level  of  behavior  will  be 
discussed  in  Chapter  II. 

There  follows  in  Table  1  a  summary  of  the  various  sources  of  data 
for  each  level  and  sublevel  of  personality.  We  should  observe  again 
that  the  assignment  of  data  to  the  appropriate  level  operates  automati- 
cally. The  source  of  the  data  routinely  and  rigidly  defines  the  level. 
It  should  also  be  noted  that  while  our  method  is  rigid,  behavior  is 
flexible  and  fluid,  and  does  not  always  follow  our  notational  schemes. 
By  this  we  mean  that  there  exist  sublevel  variations;  some  Level  II 

TABLE  1 

Operational  Definition  of  Five  Levels  of 
Personality  According  to  Source  of  Data 

Level  I:  (Public  Communication)  This  level  concerns  the  interpersonal  impact  of  the 
subject  on  others— his  social  stimulus  value.  There  are  four  different  ways  of  ob- 
taining this  measure: 

Level  I-R:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  patient's  minute-by-minute  be- 
havior in  a  social  situation. 

Level  I-S:  Sociometric  ratings  (from  check  lists)  by  fellow  patients  or  by 
trained  observers. 

Level  I-M:  MMPI  indices  which  predict  the  interpersonal  behavior  to  be 
expected. 

Level  I-T:  Scores  from  standard  situational  tests  which  assess  the  patient's  in- 
terpersonal reactions. 

I^evel  II:  (Conscious  Descriptions)  The  subject's  view  of  self  and  world  obtained  from 
interviews,  autobiography,  check  list,  questionnaire.  There  are  four  methods  which 
provide  data  for  this  level: 

Level  II-Di.  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  verbal  content  from  diagnostic 
interviews. 

Level  II-Ti:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  verbal  content  from  therapy 
interviews. 

Level  II-C:  Scores  from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  on  which  the 
patient  checks  his  view  of  self  and  others. 

Level  II-A:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  content  of  autobiographies  writ- 
ten by  patients. 

Level  III:  (Preconscious  Symbolization)  The  subject's  autistic,  projective  fantasy  pro- 
ductions. There  are  two  sublevels  of  preconscious  expression:  Level  III  Hero  and 
Level  III  Other. 


82  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

Level  III  Hero  is  defined  by  the  interpersonal  themes  attributed  to  the  heroes  of 
preconscious  protocols  obtained  from  dreams,  fantasies,  projective  stories 

Level  III  Other  comprises  the  interpersonal  themes  attributed  to  the  "other" 
figures  from  the  same  preconscious  protocols. 

There  are  at  present  seven  methods  for  collecting  Level  III  data: 

Level  III-T  Ratings  of  TAT  stories. 

Level  III-IFT:  Ratings  from  the  Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test. 

Level  III-i:  Ratings  of  responses  to  the  Iflund  pro)ective  test. 

Level  III-B:  Ratings  of  responses  to  the  Blacky  projective  test. 

Level  III-D-  Ratings  of  interpersonal  themes  in  dream  protocols. 

Level  III-F:  Ratings  of  interpersonal  themes  from  waking  fantasies  expressed 

by  the  subject. 

Level  III-M:  MMPI  indices  which  predict  preconscious  behavior. 

Level  IV:  (Unexpressed  Unconscious)  This  level  is  defined  by  two  criteria:  the  inter- 
personal themes  significantly  omitted  at  the  top  three  levels  and  significantlv  avoided 
on  tests  of  subliminal  perceptions,  selective  forgetting,  and  the  like.  Specific  methods 
for  obtaining  this  data  are  not  yet  developed. 

Level  V:  (Ego  Ideal)  This  level  comprises  the  subject's  statements  about  his  inter- 
personal ideas,  standards,  conceptions  of  good  and  evil  as  obtained  in  interview, 
autobiography,  questionnaire,  or  check  list.  There  are  three  methods  for  obtaining 
Level  V  ratings  of  values: 

Level  V-C:    Scores  from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  on  which  the 

patient  checks  his  ego  ideal. 
Level  V-Di-  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  subject's  ideals  as  expressed  in 

diagnostic  interviews. 
Level  V-Ti:  Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  subject's  ideals  as  expressed  in 

therapy  interviews. 

reports  (let  us  say  from  the  intense  confidence  of  psychotherapy) 
turn  out  to  be  much  closer  to  our  Level  III  measurements.  Some 
symbolic  productions  (Level  III)  from  subjects  who  are  striving  to 
"overload"  their  presentations  in  one  thematic  direction  may  duplicate 
Level  II  conscious  reports.  These  sublevel  shifts  are  generally  due  to 
differences  in  the  social  situation,  or  in  the  stimulus  materials,  or  gen- 
eral variability  factors  such  as  time,  oscillation,  and  interlevel  dynamics. 
All  of  these  are,  fortunately,  open  to  some  systematic  measurement 
and  predictive  control,  and  will  be  treated  in  a  later  publication. 

To  conclude  this  preliminary  glance  at  the  five  defined  levels  of 
personality,  an  illustration  of  the  way  data  are  assigned  to  levels  may 
prove  helpful.  If  a  subject  is  rated  as  displaying  aggressive  behavior 
in  a  unit  of  interpersonal  action,  the  rating  of  hostility  (E)  is  then 
coded  into  the  matrix  of  Level  I-R  variables.  Should  this  same  subject 
describe  himself  in  a  conscious  report  (on  a  check  list)  as  friendly  and 
agreeable,  a  Level  II-C  rating  of  affiliation  (M)  would  be  made. 
Should  he  report  a  dream  in  which  the  hero  behaves  in  a  submissive, 
trustful  fashion,  dependence  (K)  would  be  coded  into  the  Level  III-D 
pattern.  Should  nurturant  behavior  be  absent  from  all  of  these  three 
levels  (to  a  statistically  significant  degree),  and  if  it  appears  in  the 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  83 

form  of  exaggerated  avoidance  or  distortion  of  tenderness  themes  on 
Level  IV  measuring  devices,  then  the  presence  of  Level  IV  nurturance 
(N)  could  be  inferred.  If  his  description  of  his  "ego  ideal"  on  the 
check  list  stresses  the  themes  of  power  and  independence,  then  the 
Level  V-C  scores  of  A  and  B  are  emphasized. 

Let  us  assume  that  hundreds  of  additional  measurements  at  all  levels 
continue  to  emphasize  the  same  pattern.  The  summary  totals  for  each 
level  are  converted  to  standard  scores,  comparing  them  to  the  means 
of  appropriate  normative  larger  samples  of  cases.  By  means  of  the 
vector  method  described  above  we  can  chart  the  personality  structure 
in  the  form  of  a  diagram  summarizing  five  levels  of  self-behavior.  The 
data  from  each  level  has  been  converted  into  a  systematic  rating  lan- 
guage which  is  standardized,  and  directly  comparable  with  the  data 
from  other  levels.  The  many  implications  and  theoretical  aspects  of 
this  multidimensional  organization  of  personality  will  be  discussed  in 
Chapter  13, 

The  Measurement  of  the  Self-Other  Interaction 

A  final  notationa^  procedure  remains  before  the  basic  elements  of 
the  personality  structure  can  be  assembled.  Formal  recognition  must 
be  made  of  the  fact  that  any  interpersonal  behavior  involves  more 
than  one  person — and  by  definition  cannot  be  considered  as  an  iso- 
lated phenomenon.  We  accepted  (in  the  fourth  working  principle) 
the  premise  that  the  interpersonal  theory  logically  requires  that  for 
each  variable  or  variable  system  by  which  we  measure  the  subject's 
behavior,  we  must  include  an  equivalent  set  for  measuring  the  parallel 
behavior  of  the  subject's  interpersonal  world. 

The  reciprocal  nature  of  social  interaction,  the  reflex  way  in  which 
human  beings  tailor  their  responses  to  others,  and  the  automatic  way 
in  which  they  force  others  to  react  to  them  will  become  one  of  the 
main  points  of  emphasis  in  this  book.  To  take  systematic  account  of 
these  interchanges  (at  all  levels  of  personality)  a  notational  step  is 
required.  This  is  accomplished  by  categorizing  and  summarizing 
separately  the  interpersonal  responses  of  the  subject  and  the  specific 
others  with  whom  he  interacts.  When  we  observe  the  subject's  public 
communications  at  Level  I  we  rate  not  only  his  purposive  behavior,  but 
also  what  others  do  to  him.  Then  we  score  the  patient's  interpersonal 
responses  to  the  psychotherapist  and  we  also  score  the  latter's  reactions 
toward  the  patient.  We  note,  for  example,  that  the  subject  acts  de- 
pendent (K)  and  the  therapist  reacts  with  nurturance  (O). 

When  we  measure  the  subject's  conscious  reports  at  Level  II,  we 
rate  not  only  his  perceptions  of  himself,  but  also  his  descriptions  of 
his  interpersonal  world  as  he  views  it.  Thus  we  score  the  interpersonal 


84  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

themes  the  patient  attributes  to  himself  and,  in  addition,  the  themes 
he  attributes  to  the  specified  "others"  with  whom  he  is  concerned.  We 
rate,  for  example,  the  subject's  statement  "I  am  helpless  to  solve  this 
problem"  (/)  and  his  description  of  the  therapist  "You  are  a  person 
who  can  help  me  with  my  problem"  (O).  When  we  summarize  his 
Level  II  material,  we  obtain  a  numerical  or  diagrammatic  total  for 
the  reported  view  of  self,  his  view  of  his  therapist,  of  his  family  mem- 
bers, of  the  other  members  of  his  therapy  group,  and  all  "others"  he 
has  described. 

When  we  deal  with  the  symbolic  data  of  Level  III,  we  rate  not  only 
his  fantasy  themes  attributed  to  self  or  to  self-identified  heroes,  but 
also  the  interpersonal  themes  he  assigns  to  the  "others"  with  whom  his 
fantasy  self  interacts.  The  subject  might  report,  for  example,  a  dream 
in  which  he  attacks  (E)  his  rejecting  unsympathetic  psychothera- 
pist (C).  We  summarize  the  Level  III  fantasy  materials  in  the  same 
manner — obtaining  separate  totals  from  his  symbolic  self  and  symbolic 
others. 

The  usefulness  of  this  self-other  classification  for  the  unexpressed 
themes  of  Level  IV  is,  at  present,  an  unsettled  question.  Some  psy- 
chologists hold  that  the  vague,  diffuse  themes  from  the  less  conscious 
areas  of  personality  cannot  be  differentiated  into  self-other  categories. 
Since  there  is  no  adequate  data  to  settle  this  question,  Level  IV  behavior 
will  not  be  formally  systematized  in  this  book. 

The  division  of  behavior  into  self-and-other  does  not  seem  to  apply 
as  directly  to  the  "value"  data  from  Level  V.  It  might  be  assumed  that 
the  "ego  ideal"  or  superego  judgment  of  what's  "right-and-good" 
holds  as  a  general  value  system  for  one's  view  of  self  and  all  others. 
On  the  other  hand,  it  is  possible  to  obtain  measurements  on  the  "ideal- 
for-self"  and  the  "ideal-for-specified-others."  Thus  the  subject  might 
be  asked  to  describe  his  view  of  the  "ideal"  mother,  the  "ideal"  father, 
the  "ideal"  spouse,  the  "ideal"  therapist,  the  "ideal"  boss,  etc.  The 
Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  is  at  this  time  conducting  investi- 
gations of  this  sort,  but  the  results  are  not  yet  tabulated.  For  this 
reason  in  this  book.  Level  V  will  be  considered  as  a  unitary  field  and 
will  not  be  divided  into  self  and  other. 

Variability  Indices:  The  Organization  of  Personality 

The  interpersonal  system  deals,  therefore,  with  eight  generic  areas 
of  personality  data:  two  each  (self  and  other)  for  Levels  I,  II, 
III,  and  one  each  for  Levels  IV  and  V.  Since  Level  IV  has  been 
omitted  from  consideration  in  the  current  research,  we  shall  be  con- 
sidering in  the  following  chapters  seven  generic  areas  of  behavior.  A 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS 


85 


LEVELI 
(EGO  IDEAL) 


LEVEL  m 
(PRECONSCIOUS  EXPRESSIONS) 

Figure  4.   Schematic  Diagram  Illustrating  Seven  Generic  Areas  of  Personahtv  at 
Four  Levels  and  Listing  Some  Variability  Indices  of  Personality  Organization. 


86  BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

preliminary  diagram  of  personality  structure  can  now  be  presented. 
Figure  4  illustrates  the  generic  division  of  personality  data  with  which 
we  are  concerned.  Each  of  the  seven  circles  denotes  a  generalized  clas- 
sification of  personality  data.  Inside  each  circle  is  printed  a  suggestive 
list  of  the  sources  of  data  for  each  level.  This  is  a  schematic,  pictorial 
representation  summarizing  the  notational  procedures  thus  far  out- 
lined. It  is  highly  generalized.  As  it  stands  here  it  could  not  be  used 
for  clinical  or  research  purposes  since  only  one  circle  for  "others"  is 
represented.  In  practice  we  would  have  as  many  "others"  circles  as 
necessary  to  summarize  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  each  person  with 
whom  the  subject  interacts. 

More  practical  and  accurate  working  diagrams  are  presented  in  the 
clinical  and  diagnostic  chapters  to  follow.  The  stylized  diagram  in 
Figure  4  is  presented  to  point  up  the  next  organizational  issue,  the  re- 
lationship among  the  levels  of  personality.  It  will  be  noted  that  a  series 
of  lines  connects  the  seven  circles  in  Figure  4.  These  represent  the 
dynamic  interactions  among  the  levels.  Each  circle,  it  will  be  remem- 
bered, summarizes  the  pattern  of  standard  scores  on  the  same  matrix  of 
sixteen  variables.  We  can,  therefore,  make  direct  mathematical  com- 
parison between  levels.  The  line  between  Level  II  Self  and  Level  III 
Self  stands  for  the  subtractive  comparison  between  the  two  areas  of 
personality,  and  indicates  how  similar  or  different  they  are.  In  addi- 
tion, it  provides  a  numerical  statement  of  those  interpersonal  themes 
which  appear  in  private  symbolization  and  which  are  not  consciously 
attributed  to  self.  These  relationships  among  levels  are  called  intra- 
personal  variability  indices.  They  are,  in  some  respects,  operational 
redefinitions  of  certain  Freudian  "defense  mechanisms,"  since  they 
systematically  summarize  the  comparisons  among  the  levels  of  per- 
sonality. The  Freudians  call  these  interlevel  relationships  "defense 
mechanisms"  because  they  are  seen  as  "warding  off"  instinctual  im- 
pulses. We  have,  however,  in  principle  hesitated  to  accept  this  focus- 
ing on  the  unconscious  level  of  behavior  and  have  accepted  instead  an 
emphasis  on  the  over-all  organization  of  all  levels.  We  have  tended  to 
see  these  interlevel  relationships  simply  as  indices  which  reflect  the 
structure  of  personality  organization  and  the  kind  and  amount  of  con- 
flict, or  rigidity,  or  flexibility. 

We  therefore  call  these  relationships  among  the  areas  of  behavior 
variability  indices.  They  reflect  in  mathematical  terms  the  tendency 
of  any  one  level  to  dupUcate  or  balance  the  inevitable  distortions  of 
the  other  levels  of  personality.  The  definition,  meaning,  and  function 
of  these  generic  mechanisms  of  organization,  will  be  discussed  in  Part 
III  of  this  book. 


INTERPERSONAL  AND  VARIABILITY  SYSTEMS  87 

Summary 

This  chapter,  by  way  of  overview  and  prospectus,  has  presented  a 
classificatory  system  for  ordering  interpersonal  behavior.  Five  levels 
at  which  this  behavior  exists  have  been  defined.  A  brief  survey  of  the 
system  of  variability  indices  which  link  together  these  levels  has  been 
included. 

The  subsequent  chapters  will  be  devoted  to  a  detailed  fiUing-in  of 
the  broad  areas  outlined  in  this  chapter.  The  next  six  chapters  will 
deal  with  the  levels  of  personality — theory,  measurement,  method- 
ology, and  meaning  of  the  varieties  of  interpersonal  behavior.  Chap- 
ter 1 3  will  deal  with  the  variability  dimension — and  will  present  defi- 
nitions of  the  specific  interlevel  relations  involved  in  the  formal  nota- 
tional  system. 

References 

1.  Blum,  G.  S.  The  Blacky  Pictures:  A  technique  for  the  exploration  of  personality 
dynamics.  New  York.  The  Psychological  CoqD.,  1950. 

2.  Freud,  S.  Why  war?  In  Collected  Papers.  Translated  by  James  Strachey.  Vol. 
S.   London:  Hogarth  Press  and  Institute  of  Psychoanalysis,  1950. 

3.  Iflund,  B.  Selective  recall  of  meaningless  materials  as  related  to  psychoanalytic 
formulations  in  certain  psychiatric  syndromes.  Unpublished  doctor's  dissertauon. 
University  of  Calif orma,  Berkeley,  1953. 

4.  LaForge,  R.,  M.  Freedman,  T.  Leary,  H.  Naboisek,  and  H.  Coffey.  The  inter- 
personal dimension  of  personality:  II  An  objective  study  of  repression.  /.  Pers., 
1954,  23,  No.  2,  129-53. 

5.  LaForge,  R.,  and  R.  Suczek.  The  interpersonal  dimension  of  personahty.  Ill  An 
interpersonal  checklist.  /.  Pers.,  1955,  24,  No.  1,  94-112. 

6.  Lodge,  G.  T.,  and  C.  J.  Steenbarger.  Charting  the  course  of  the  Rorschach  inter- 
view.  /.  Gen.  Psychol.,  1953,  48,  67-73. 

7.  Miller,  Milton  L.  The  traumatic  effects  of  surgical  operations  in  childhood  on 
the  interpretive  functions  of  the  ego.    Psych.  Quart.,  1950,  20,  77-92. 

8.  Naboisek,  H.  Interpersonal  assessments  of  patients  in  group  therapy.  Unpub- 
lished doctor's  dissertation.  University  of  California,  Berkeley,  1953. 

9.  Parsons,  T.,  and  R.  F.  Bales.  The  dimensions  of  action-space.  In  Working 
papers  in  the  theory  of  action.    Glencoe,  111.-  The  Free  Press,  1953. 

10.  Reichenbach,  H.  R.  The  rise  of  scientific  philosophy.  University  of  California 
Press,  1951. 

11.  Stagner,  Ross.  Psychology  of  personality.  New  York-  McGraw,  1937. 


II 


The  Interpersonal  Dimension  of  Personality: 
Variables,  Levels,  and  Diagnostic  Categories 


Introduction 

The  following  section  of  this  book  is  devoted  to  a  discussion  of  inter- 
personal behavior  at  five  levels  of  personality.  These  levels  are: 

I.  The  Level  of  Public  Communication 

II.  The  Level  of  Conscious  Communication 

III.  The  Level  of  Private  Communication 

IV.  The  Level  of  the  Unexpressed 
V.  The  Level  of  Values 

A  chapter  is  devoted  to  each  of  these  levels.  Each  chapter  in- 
cludes a  historical  review  of  previous  theories  relating  to  the  level  in 
question,  an  operational  definition  of  the  level,  a  system  for  measuring 
behavior  at  the  level,  and  a  discussion  of  its  significance.  Considerable 
theoretical  speculation  as  to  the  meaning  of  behavior  at  each  level  will 
be  included.  In  order  to  preserve  the  descriptive  and  theoretical  orien- 
tation of  the  book  we  have  not  included  a  detailed  account  of  the  re- 
search findings.  Where  there  is  evidence  supporting  these  speculations, 
reference  will  be  made  to  the  scientific  publication  in  which  the  perti- 
nent research  has  been  described. 

This  section  is  concluded  by  a  description  of  the  system  of  inter- 
personal diagnosis  (Chapter  12).  Here  we  employ  the  data  from  three 
levels  of  behavior  to  construct  an  objective  multilevel  diagnostic  sys- 
tem. 


90 


7 


The  Level  of  Public  Communication: 
The  Interpersonal  Reflex 


This  chapter  takes  as  its  subject  interpersonal  communication.  This 
aspect  of  personality,  which  we  have  designated  Level  I,  is  concerned 
with  the  social  impact  that  one  human  being  has  on  another.  We  shall 
consider  first  some  methods  for  isolating  and  defining  these  interac- 
tions, and  then  proceed  to  their  impHcations  for  personahty  theory. 

The  events  studied  at  this  level  are  the  overt  interpersonal  activities 
of  the  individual.  What  a  person  does  in  any  social  situation  is  a  func- 
tion of  at  least  two  factors,  ( 1 )  his  multilevel  personality  structure  and 
(2)  the  activities  and  effect  of  the  "other  one,"  the  person  with  whom 
he  is  interacting. 

In  order  to  define  and  to  discuss  the  level  of  overt  communication 
it  is  necessary  at  times  to  tear  it  out  of  these  two  broader  contexts  in 
which  it  is  always  imbedded.  The  criteria  of  logical  narration  demand 
that  we  talk  about  interpersonal  behavior  in  this  chapter  as  though  it 
exists  apart  from  the  other  aspects  of  the  person's  personality  struc- 
ture or  apart  from  the  behavior  of  others.  These  broader  contexts  are 
always  implicitly  referred  to  and  should  be  kept  in  mind. 

Definitions  and  Illustrations 

The  basic  unit  involved  here  is  the  interpersonal  effect.  We  de- 
termine the  interpersonal  meaning  of  any  behavior  by  asking,  "What 
is  this  person  doing  to  the  other?  What  kind  of  a  relationship  is  he 
attempting  to  establish  through  this  panicular  behavior?"  The  an- 
swers to  these  questions  define  the  subject's  interpersonal  impact  on 
the  other  one.  For  example,  "He  is  boasting  and  attempting  to  estab- 
lish superiority";  or,  "He  is  rejecting  and  refusing  to  help." 

We  are  concerned  at  this  level  with  ivhat  one  person  communi- 
cates to  another.  A  father,  for  example,  may  employ  one  or  one  thou- 

9» 


^2  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

sand  words  to  refuse  his  child's  request.  The  mode,  style,  and  con- 
tent of  the  two  rejecting  expressions  may  be  very  different,  but  their 
interpersonal  effect  is  the  same — rejection. 

In  studying  the  interpersonal  purposes  which  underlie  human  be- 
havior, the  following  hypothesis  has  developed.  It  seems  that  in  a 
large  percentage  of  interactions  the  basic  motives  are  expressed  in  a 
reflex  manner.  They  are  so  automatic  that  they  are  often  unwitting 
and  often  at  variance  with  the  subject's  own  perception  of  them.  This 
facet  of  behavior  is  therefore  a  difficult  one  to  isolate  and  measure.  It 
is  often  unverbalized  and  so  subtle  and  reflex  as  to  escape  articulate 
description.  Sometimes  these  interpersonal  communications  can  be 
implicit  in  the  content  of  the  discussion:  Grandfather  talks  incessantly 
about  the  lack  of  energy  and  initiative  of  modern  youth  in  order  to 
impress  others  with  the  fact  that  he  is  a  successful,  self-made  man. 
Grandmother  talks  incessantly  about  sickness,  calamity,  and  death  to 
remind  others  that  the  time  may  be  short  to  repay  her  for  the  sacrifices 
she  has  made  for  her  children.  Grandfather  never  says  openly,  "I  am 
better  than  you  young  people."  Grandmother  never  says,  "You 
should  feel  guilty  and  devoted  to  me."  Grandfather's  remark  may  be 
concerned  with  the  issue  of  the  40-hour  week.  Grandmother  may  be 
quoting  from  the  obituary  column  of  the  evening  paper.  Behind  the 
superficial  content  of  these  expressions  are  the  repetitive  interpersonal 
motives — superiority  and  reproach.  Behind  the  superficial  content  of 
most  social  exchanges  it  is  possible  to  determine  the  naked  motive  com- 
munications: I  am  wise;  I  am  strong;  I  am  friendly;  I  am  contemptuous; 
as  well  as  the  concomitant  messages:  you  are  less  wise,  less  strong, 
likable,  contemptible.  Jung  has  described  the  "persona"  as  a  mask-like 
front  behind  which  more  basic  motives  exist.  The  purposive  behavior 
we  are  dealing  with  in  this  chapter  is  similar,  but  in  emphasis  something 
more  important  than  just  a  social  facade.  It  is  closer,  perhaps,  to  the 
"character  armour"  concept  from  the  earlier  writings  of  Wilhelm 
Reich,  in  that  it  assumes  a  major  role  in  the  personaUty  organization. 
Its  relationship  to  the  "conversation  of  gestures"  developed  by  Mead 
is,  as  we  shall  see,  quite  close.  Let  us  examine  some  examples  of  Level 
I  interpersonal  communications. 

How  A  Poignant  Woman  Provokes  a  Helpful  Attitude.  A  pa- 
tient comes  to  a  psychiatrist  for  an  evaluation  interview.  She  reports 
a  long  list  of  symptoms — insomnia,  worry,  depression — and  a  list  of 
unfortunate  events — divorce,  unsympathetic  employer,  etc.  She  cries. 
Whether  her  expressions  are  scored  separately  and  summarized  or 
judged  on  the  over-all,  we  derive  a  clear  picture  of  a  JK  approach — "I 
am  weak,  unhappy,  unlucky,  in  need  of  your  help." 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  93 

Let  us  shift  now  to  the  psychiatrist.  He  is  under  strong  pressure  to 
express  sympathetic,  nurturant  communications.  Helpless,  trustful  be- 
havior tends  to  pull  assistance;  that  is,  JK  tends  to  provoke  ON  from 
the  other  one.  Further,  the  patient-therapist  situation  is  in  essence  one 
that  lends  itself  easily  to  the  "needs  help-offers  help"  relationship. 
There  exists  a  tendency  for  the  psychiatrist  to  express  openly  (or 
much  more  likely,  by  implication)  that  he  knows  of  a  way  by  which 
the  patient  can  be  assisted.  This  may  be  communicated,  not  in  ivhat  he 
says,  but  in  his  bearing,  attitude,  his  very  quiet  competence. 

What  makes  it  more  complex  is  the  fact  that  the  verbal  expression 
may  be  quite  different  from  the  actual  developing  relationship.  The 
psychiatrist  may  interpret  the  dangers  of  dependence  and  the  necessity 
for  self-help.  The  patient  may  agree.  If  both  parties  tend  to  over- 
emphasize verbal  symbols,  there  may  be  an  illusion  that  a  collaborative 
relationship  exists.  Actually,  the  "nurturant  interpreter-trustful  fol- 
lower" situation  still  exists,  not  in  what  the  participants  are  saying, 
but  in  what  they  are  doing  to  each  other. 

How  THE  Penitentiary  Trains  the  Prisoner  for  Criminal  Ag- 
gression. Many  institutional  or  cultural  situations  have  interpersonal 
implications  so  built  into  them  that  a  flexible,  collaborative  relation- 
ship is  impossible.  In  prison  psychiatry,  for  example,  as  analyzed  by 
Powelson  and  Bendix  (8),  it  is  virtually  impossible  for  the  doctor  as 
well  as  for  the  patient  to  shake  off  the  institution's  implicit  punitive 
contempt  for  the  inmate.  The  penitentiary  administration  tells  the 
prisoner,  by  the  prison  architecture,  the  structure  of  the  guard-inmate 
relationship,  and  by  every  nonverbal  cue  possible  that  he  is  a  danger- 
ous, evil,  untrustworthy  outcast.  The  prisoner  often  responds  to  this 
interpersonal  pressure  by  accepting  the  role  he  is  being  trained  for. 
That  is  BCD  pulls  EFG.  The  same  interpersonal  connotations  were 
typical  of  the  descriptive  preanalytic  psychiatry  of  the  last  century 
and  of  incarcerative  psychiatry  of  the  present.  Here  we  must  note 
again  that  human  relationships  are  never  one-sided  and  that  those 
which  are  rigid  or  of  long  duration  tend  to  be  selective  on  both  sides. 
Thus,  as  Powelson  has  pointed  out,  the  recidivist  criminal  is  least 
anxious  when  he  is  in  passive  rebellion  against  a  strong  punitive  author- 
ity who  feeds  him  and  beats  him. 

Interpersonal  Implications  Underlie  Social  Organizations. 
To  a  lesser  degree  any  doctor-patient  relationship  tends  to  have  pre- 
determined interpersonal  structure.  At  least  at  the  beginning  it  is 
highly  loaded  by  the  dependence-helpfulness  axis. 

Unverbalized  interpersonal  assumptions  tend  to  pervade  every  so- 
cial organization.   The  unwitting  evaluation  of  the  differing  roles  of 


94  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

orderly,  nurse,  psychotherapist,  psychiatrist,  and  administrator  in 
relationship  to  each  other  is  an  inevitable  phenomenon  in  any  psychia- 
tric hospital.  The  way  in  which  eddies  from  these  power  whirlpools 
reach  and  relate  to  the  patient  is  probably  more  important  in  terms  of 
the  remission  rate  than  the  number  of  electric  shock  machines  or  the 
skill  of  the  psychotherapists.  Factory,  department  store,  office,  uni- 
versity— all  have  these  complex  networks  of  routine,  unverbalized 
evaluation  through  which  power,  prestige,  contempt,  punishment,  ac- 
ceptance, etc.,  are  expressed.  Systematic  understanding  of  these  social 
hierarchies  and  their  effect  on  the  clients,  patients,  employees,  cus- 
tomers, and  students  is  a  problem  for  the  sociologist  or  the  industrial 
psychologist.  Investigations  in  these  areas  will  very  likely  reveal  that 
individuals  tend  to  select  jobs  and  occupational  roles  in  accordance 
with  their  interpersonal  techniques  for  anxiety  reduction.  We  con- 
sider these  phenomena  here  because  they  demonstrate  the  implicit  and 
automatic  nature  of  interpersonal  reactivity. 

How  THE  Professor  and  Student  Train  Each  Other  To  Be 
Professor  and  Student.  The  teacher-student  relationship,  obviously 
loaded  with  power  implications,  serves  to  illustrate  some  details  of 
reflex  communication.  Professors  are  so  addicted  to  the  stereotyped 
teaching  reflex  that  they  often  cannot  inhibit  the  didactic  response. 
We  recall  the  psychology  professor  who  had  developed  at  some  length 
in  a  lecture  the  thesis  that  teachers  or  psychotherapists  should  not  give 
answers  but  should  stimulate  the  student  or  the  patient  to  seek  answers 
himself.  "Don't  let  them  become  dependent  on  you;  make  them  think 
for  themselves."  As  soon  as  the  lecture  was  over,  a  graduate  student 
(well  trained  to  the  dependency  reflex)  rushed  up  with  a  question:  "In 
my  undergraduate  teaching  section  the  students  are  continually  ask- 
ing me  to  solve  their  personal  problems  and  demanding  answers.  What 
shall  I  do?"  Pausing  only  to  clear  his  throat,  the  professor  reflexly 
responded:  "Yes,  you'll  always  find  your  students  tending  to  trap  you 
into  solving  their  problems  for  them — the  problems  that  they  should 
work  out  for  themselves.  Now  what  I'd  do  if  I  were  you  is,  first,  I'd 
get  them  to.  .  .  ."  The  verbal  content  of  an  interaction  can  be  quite 
divorced  from  the  interpersonal  meaning. 

These  subtle,  ubiquitous,  automatic  role  relationships  have  as  their 
function  the  minimization  of  anxiety.  They  set  up  smooth-flowing 
reciprocal  interactions  of  ask-teach,  attack-defend,  etc.  On  those  oc- 
casions when  the  pattern  of  interpersonal  reflexes  breaks  down  or  is 
ambiguous,  considerable  distress  generally  results — manifested  in  the 
accustomed  symptoms  of  anxiousness.  Some  students  are  made  un- 
comfortable by  a  teacher  who  refuses  to  lecture  and  assume  the 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


95 


authoritative  role.  Patients  often  manifest  initial  bewilderment  and 
insecurity  when  the  therapist  appears  disinterested  in  giving  quick 
answers  to  their  problems.  Symbiotic  marriage  partnerships  can  be 
thrown  into  panic  when  the  implicit  assumptions  of  power,  guilt,  and 
dependence  on  which  they  rest  are  temporarily  threatened. 

So  far  we  have  viewed  interpersonal  communications  as  automatic 
responses  in  standard  institutional  situations.  We  shall  now  proceed 
to  study  them  in  the  context  of  the  individual  personality,  in  terms 
of  the  classification  system  of  16  variables  presented  in  the  last 
chapter. 

The  preliminary  data  on  which  this  system  is  based  was  obtained 
by  giving  extensive  psychological  test  batteries  to  some  200  subjects 
and  then  recording  their  interactions  in  45  discussion  or  therapy 
groups.  The  pretesting  procedures  produced  many  ratings  of  Level 
II  (conscious  perception)  and  Level  III  (symboUc  productions)  be- 
havior. The  Level  I-R  communications  were  obtained  by  studying 
the  subjects'  behavior  as  they  interacted  with  the  four  or  five  other 
group  members.  Let  us  select  one  subject  as  an  example  and  follow 
him  through  this  procedure. 

How  A  Sullen  Patient  Teaches  Others  to  Reject  Him.  A 
thirty-year-old  man  came  to  the  psychiatric  clinic  with  complaints  of 
depression,  general  immobilization,  and  social  isolation.  After  intake 
interviews  and  testing,  he  entered  a  psychotherapy  group  along  with 
four  other  patients.  All  the  group  members  were  strangers  when  they 
met. 


SUBJECT 


FELLOW   GROUP   MEMBERS 


Figure  5.   Summary  of  Interpersonal  Interactions  Between  an  Illustrative  Subject 
and  Four  Fellow  Group  Members. 


96  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

The  verbal  transactions  of  the  group  were  recorded  and  transcribed. 
Psychologists  then  rated  each  speech  which  this  patient  made  and  each 
verbal  reaction  by  other  patients  to  him.  The  sixteen-point  circular 
continuum  of  variables  (presented  in  the  preceding  chapter)  was  em- 
ployed in  these  ratings.  The  interpersonal  actions  of  the  subject  and 
the  reactions  of  the  fellow  group  members  during  the  first  eight  ses- 
sions were  then  summarized,  combined  into  octants,  and  plotted  on  a 
circular  profile.  These  Level  I-R  "self"  and  "other"  profiles  are  pre- 
sented in  Figure  5. 

This  diagram  tells  us  that  the  subject  acted  in  a  bitter,  distrustful 
manner  (FGH)  in  a  group.  He  complained,  demanded,  accused, 
withdrew.  His  fellow  patients  reacted  to  him  with  a  critical,  un- 
sympathetic, rejecting  exasperation  (CDE).  After  eight  sessions  in 
the  group  the  patient  had  virtually  duplicated  the  suspicious,  isolated 
pattern  that  had  originally  brought  him  to  the  cUnic.  This  rather  pure 
and  didactically  simple  interpersonal  situation  serves  to  illustrate  sev- 
eral interesting  aspects  of  interpersonal  theory  which  will  now  be 
considered. 

The  Interpersonal  Reflex 

First  we  ask,  what  did  this  patient  do  to  get  four  strangers  to  agree 
on  his  social  stimulus  value?  It  seems  that  he  trained  them  to  react  to 
him  in  a  very  specific  way — provoking  them  to  rejection  and  irrita- 
tion. This  question  becomes  more  important  (from  the  diagnostic 
viewpoint)  when  we  remember  that  he  reports  that  over  the  span  of 
his  life  he  has  consistently  tended  to  remain  isolated  and  despised  by 
others.  How  does  he  do  this?  He  made,  on  the  average,  ten  verbal 
comments  in  each  group  session.  What  happened  in  these  eight  meet- 
ings of  the  group  to  bring  about  a  significant  disaffiliation? 

What  Are  Interpersonal  Reflexes?  When  we  trace  his  inter- 
personal actions  back  to  the  original  recorded  protocols  we  discover 
that  a  typical  pattern  of  Level  I  interaction  existed.  The  individual 
units  of  this  behavior  we  call  interpersonal  ?nechanisms  or  interpersonal 
reflexes.  They  are  defined  as  the  observable,  expressive  units  of  face- 
to-face  social  behavior. 

These  reflexes  are  automatic  and  usually  involuntary  responses  to 
interpersonal  situations.  They  are  often  independent  of  the  content 
of  the  communication.  They  are  the  individual's  spontaneous  methods 
of  reacting  to  others. 

The  exact  manner  in  which  these  Level  I  communications  are  ex- 
pressed is  a  complex  problem.  This  much  is  clear:  they  are  expressed 
partly  in  the  content  or  verbal  meaning  of  the  communication,  but 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  97 

primarily  in  the  tone  of  voice,  gesture,  carriage,  and  external  appear- 
ance. Although  we  do  not  know  the  specific  method  by  which  human 
beings  communicate  their  emotional  messages  to  each  other,  we  can 
rate  with  reliable  confidence  the  over-all,  molar  effect.  Raters  (trained 
psychologists  or  untrained  fellow  patients)  can  agree  with  impressive 
reliability  in  rating  what  subjects  do  to  each  other  in  interpersonal  sit- 
uations. Preliminary  research  by  Blanche  Sweet  (10)  suggests  that 
listening  to  recordings  leads  to  more  effective  ratings  than  reading 
typed  transcriptions.  Sound  movies  would  provide  the  optimal  tech- 
niques for  preserving  the  nuances  involved  in  interpersonal  reflexes. 
Future  research  may  determine  the  specific  way  in  which  these  spon- 
taneous interpersonal  meanings  manifest  themselves  to  others.  The 
reflex  manner  in  which  human  beings  react  to  others  and  train  others 
to  respond  to  them  in  selective  ways  is,  I  believe,  the  most  important 
single  aspect  of  personality.  The  systematic  estimates  of  a  patient's 
repertoire  of  interpersonal  reflexes  is  a  key  factor  in  functional  diag- 
nosis. Awareness  and,  if  possible,  modification  of  crippled  or  mal- 
adaptive reflexes  should  be  a  basic  step  in  psychotherapy.  When  more 
evidence  as  to  the  mode  of  expression — gesture,  carriage,  content  of 
speech — is  at  hand,  some  additions  to  therapeutic  practice  may 
develop. 

The  automatic  and  involuntary  nature  of  interpersonal  reflexes 
makes  them  difficult  to  observe  and  measure  by  a  participant  in  any 
interaction.  They  are,  for  the  same  reason,  most  resistant  to  thera- 
peutic change.  The  more  the  members  of  the  psychotherapy  group 
tried  to  explain  to  the  subject  how  and  why  he  irritated  them,  the  more 
he  protested  his  feelings  of  injury.  Later,  intellectual  insight  and 
voluntary  controlled  changes  to  cooperative,  self-confident  behavior 
developed.  These  were,  however,  quite  tentative  and  unnatural.  Dur- 
ing many  months  of  treatment  spontaneous  reactivity  brought  a 
return  of  the  original  responses.  This  involves,  of  course,  the  familiar 
process  of  "working  through,"  basic  to  most  therapeutic  enterprises. 

Physiological  and  Interpersonal  Reflexes.  The  on-going  in- 
voluntary nature  of  these  reflexes  demands  continual  emphasis  to  keep 
them  from  slipping  out  of  focus.  This  is  the  hidden  dimension  of  be- 
havior. This  is  the  area  of  personality  which  it  never  occurs  to  us  to 
mention,  so  basic  that  it  is  taken  for  granted.  Consider  this  analogy: 
A  physician  conducting  a  medical  examination  interview  may  ask  the 
patient  to  report  any  physiological  events  he  may  have  noticed  during 
the  previous  day.  The  patient  might  describe  the  heavy  feeling  in  his 
stomach  after  lunch,  the  headache  during  the  evening.  It  would  not 
occur  to  the  patient  to  recall  that  he  automatically  blinked  his  eyes  on 


98 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


the  average  of  three  times  a  minute,  180  times  an  hour,  2,880  times 
during  the  16  hours  of  a  waking  day.  Nor  would  the  patient  ordi- 
narily be  able  to  report  the  absence  of  a  reflex.  He  might  describe  the 
symptom  that  accompanies  it,  but  the  presence  or  absence  of  physical 
reflexes  is  generally  unnoticed  by  the  patient.  His  failure  to  mention 
them  in  the  medical  interview  is,  of  course,  natural  and  proper.  It  is 
not  a  sign  of  malignant  "repressive"  mechanisms  but  rather  of  the 
implicit  nature  of  these  important  behaviors. 

Compare  this,  now,  with  the  psychiatric  interview.  If  asked  to  re- 
port the  pertinent  psychological  events  of  the  previous  day,  the  patient 
might  remember  the  feeling  of  depression  in  the  forenoon,  rage  at  the 
office,  and  worry  over  bills  at  home  in  the  evening.  It  is  inconceivable 
that  he  would  or  could  recount  that  in  almost  every  interpersonal  situa- 
tion he  conveyed  by  gesture,  bearing,  tone  of  voice,  and  the  negativism 
of  his  verbalization  a  consistent  message  of  pessimism  and  resentment, 
that  over  70  per  cent  of  his  interpersonal  mechanisms  were  in  the  same 
direction,  that  the  "others"  with  whom  he  regularly  interacts  have 
been  trained  to  respond  to  him  in  an  irritated  and  rejecting  manner. 
Nor  would  he  indicate  that  the  interpersonal  reflexes  expressing  ten- 
der or  affiliative  purposes  are  crippled  and  inhibited.  Again,  his  failure 
to  mention  these  involuntary  actions  is  not  a  symptom  or  a  pathologi- 
cal repressive  maneuver.  The  reflexes  which  we  measure  at  Level  I 
tend  to  operate  as  background  to  the  verbal  content  of  the  communi- 
cation. It  is  the  latter  to  which  we  consciously  attend;  but  it  is  the 
former  which  set  the  tone  and  provide  the  interpersonal  significance  of 
the  event. 

Interpersonal  reflexes  are  considerably  more  tricky  to  deal  with 
than  their  physical  analogues.  The  medical  examiner  has  routine, 
straightforward  methods  for  checking  physiological  reflexes.  The 
psychological  situation  is  not  so  simple.  The  therapist  may  have  to 
examine  his  own  reflexive  responses  to  the  patient  with  great  care 
before  he  can  detect  the  exasperated  boredom  or  irritation  that  this 
type  of  patient  can  pull  from  others. 

The  Interpersonal  Reflex  Need  Not  Be  Conscious.  In  this 
chapter  we  are  dealing  with  the  level  of  interpersonal  action.  In  the 
following  chapter  we  shall  define  conscious  description  as  being  a  dif- 
ferent level  of  behavior.  Level  I  is  what  the  subject  does.  Level  II 
is  what  he  says  he  does. 

The  interpersonal  reflex  is,  therefore,  not  necessarily  a  conscious 
expression.  It  can  be  involuntary  and  not  a  deliberate  or  conscious 
performance. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  99 

This  difference  has  been  noted  by  other  writers.  Mead  (7,  p.  18) 
points  out  the  difference  between  gestural  behavior  (Level  1)  and 
consciousness  (Level  II).  "The  mechanism  of  the  social  act  can  be 
traced  out  without  introducing  into  it  the  conception  of  consciousness 
as  a  separable  element  within  that  act;  hence  the  social  act,  in  its  more 
elementary  stages  or  forms,  is  possible  without,  or  apart  from,  some 
form  of  consciousness."  Cassirer  (4,  p.  53)  has  made  the  same  distinc- 
tion: 

Speech  is  not  a  simple  and  uniform  phenomenon.  It  consists  of  different  ele- 
ments which,  both  biologically  and  systematically,  are  not  on  the  same  level. 
We  must  try  to  find  the  order  and  interrelationships  of  the  constituent  ele- 
ments; we  must,  as  it  were,  distinguish  the  various  geological  strata  of  speech. 
The  first  and  most  fundamental  stratum  is  evidently  the  language  of  the  emo- 
tions. A  great  portion  of  all  human  utterance  still  belongs  to  this  stratum.  But 
there  is  a  form  of  speech  that  shows  us  quite  a  different  type.  Here  the  word 
is  by  no  means  a  mere  interjection;  it  is  not  an  involuntary  expression  of  feeling, 
but  a  part  of  a  sentence  which  has  a  definite  syntactical  and  logical  structure. 
It  is  true  that  even  in  highly  developed,  in  theoretical  language  the  connection 
with  the  first  element  is  not  entirely  broken  off.  Scarcely  a  sentence  can  be 
found— except  perhaps  the  pure  formal  sentences  of  mathematics— without  a 
certain  affective  or  emotional  tinge. 

The  thesis  of  the  present  work  is  in  agreement  with  these  two 
authors.  It  is  possible  to  express  interpersonal  behavior  of  which  one 
is  not  aware.  This  is  not  to  say  that  social  reflex  behavior  is  to  be 
equated  with  the  classic  "unconscious."  We  are  speaking  instead  of  an 
involuntary,  automatic  behavior  of  which  the  subject  can  or  cannot  be 
aware. 

Previous  Literature  on  Interpersonal  Communication.  In 
making  interpersonal  communication  a  key  concept  in  the  present 
theory  of  personality,  we  are  by  no  means  introducing  a  new  planet 
into  the  constellation  of  personality  processes.  The  importance  of 
reflex  interactive  behavior  has  been  long  recognized  by  sociologists 
and  anthropologists. 

The  psychologist-philosopher  George  H.  Mead  made  a  similar  no- 
tion the  kfeystone  of  his  "social  behaviorism."  Mead  has  traced  in  great 
detail  the  development  of  human  communication,  and  he  discussed 
many  concepts  which  are  directly  related  to  the  interpersonal  system 
developed  in  this  book.  While  space  does  not  permit  the  detailed 
analysis  which  Mead's  work  deserves,  it  might  be  useful  to  note  some 
of  the  concepts  which  are  directly  related  to  Level  I  communications. 
Mead  places  the  origin  of  communication  in  the  "conversation  of  ges- 
tures" which,  as  he  defines  it,  is  very  close  to  the  definition  of  Level  I 


lOo  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

used  in  this  book.  "We  are  reading  the  meaning  of  the  conduct  of 
other  people  when,  perhaps,  they  are  not  aware  of  it.  There  is  some- 
thing that  reveals  to  us  what  the  purpose  is — just  the  glance  of  an  eye, 
the  attitude  of  the  body  which  leads  to  the  response.  The  communi- 
cation set  up  in  this  way  between  individuals  may  be  very  perfect. 
Conversation  in  gestures  may  be  carried  on  which  cannot  be  trans- 
lated into  articulate  speech."  (7,  p.  14)  He  continues  in  the  same 
section  to  say:  "But  if  we  are  going  to  broaden  the  concept  of  language 
in  the  sense  I  have  spoken  of,  so  that  it  takes  in  the  underlying  attitudes, 
we  can  see  that  the  so-called  intent,  the  idea  we  are  talking  about  is 
one  that  is  involved  in  the  gesture  or  attitudes  which  we  are  using. 
The  offering  of  a  chair  to  a  person  who  comes  into  the  room  is  in  itself 
a  courteous  act.  We  do  not  have  to  assume  that  a  person  says  to  him- 
self that  this  person  wants  a  chair.  The  offering  of  a  chair  by  a  person 
of  good  manners  is  something  which  is  almost  instinctive.  This  is  the 
very  attitude  of  the  individual.  From  the  point  of  view  of  the  observer 
it  is  a  gesture.  Such  early  stages  of  social  acts  precede  the  symbol 
proper,  and  deliberate  communication."  (7,  p.  15) 

From  this  passage  we  see  that  the  conversation  of  gestures  (which 
we  call  reflex  communication)  is,  for  Mead,  a  lower  order  of  behavior. 
As  he  develops  his  theory  of  the  "significant  symbol"  he  tends  to  de- 
preciate the  importance  of  reflex,  automatic  (nonconscious)  com- 
munication. When  he  compares  it  with  vocal,  self-conscious,  reflexive 
language,^  this  becomes  quite  apparent:  "When,  now,  that  gesture 
means  this  idea  behind  it  and  it  arouses  that  idea  in  the  other  individual, 
then  we  have  a  significant  symbol.  In  the  case  of  the  dog-fight 
(Mead's  example  of  Level  I,  gestural  communication),  we  have  a  ges- 
ture which  calls  out  appropriate  response;  in  the  present  case  we  have 
a  symbol  which  answers  to  a  meaning  in  the  experience  of  the  first 
individual  and  which  also  calls  out  that  meaning  in  the  second  indi- 
vidual. Where  the  gesture  reaches  that  situation  it  has  become  what  we 

'  Language  usage  becomes  tricky  at  this  point.  When  Mead  uses  the  term  reftexive, 
he  means  somethmg  quite  different  from  the  term  reflex  as  used  in  this  book.  He 
states:  "It  is  by  means  of  reflexiveness— the  turning  back  of  the  experience  of  the 
individual  upon  himself  that  the  whole  social  process  is  thus  brought  into  the  experi- 
ence of  the  individuals  involved  in  it;  it  is  by  such  means,  which  enable  the  individual 
to  take  the  attitude  of  the  other  toward  himself,  that  the  individual  is  able  consciously 
to  adjust  himself  to  that  process,  and  to  modify  the  resultant  of  that  process  in  any 
given  social  act  in  terms  of  his  adjustment  to  it.  Reflexiveness,  then,  is  the  essential 
condition,  within  the  social  process,  for  the  development  of  mind."  (7,  p.  134)  This 
terminology  contrasts  with  that  used  in  this  book.  The  interpersonal  behavior  at 
Level  I  which  is  (or  at  least,  can  be)  nonconscious,  involuntary,  gestural,  which 
involves  an  automatic  communication  with  or  "training  of"  the  other  one  we  call 
reflex.  The  variable  by  which  we  measure  Level  I  behavior  is  the  interpersonal  reflex, 
or  the  interpersonal  mechanism. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  loi 

call  "language."  It  is  now  a  significant  symbol  and  it  signifies  a  cer- 
tain meaning."  (7,  p.  45) 

Notice  in  this  quotation  how  Mead  distinguishes  between  the  non- 
conscious  language  of  gestures  and  the  highly  conscious  significant 
symbol.  The  latter  is  a  high-order  concept  and  from  the  systematic 
point  of  view  involves  three  separate  levels  of  personality.  Mead's 
purpose  in  developing  a  social  theory  of  mind  led  him  to  employ  com- 
plex combinations  of  personality  variables.  This  is  quite  justifiable 
from  the  standpoint  of  Mead's  conceptual  intentions,  but  prevents  a 
direct  comparison  to  the  systematic  definition  of  levels  which  we  are 
attempting  in  this  book.  In  the  broader  sense.  Mead's  social  behavior- 
ism can  rightly  be  considered  the  creative  watershed  to  which  later 
theories  of  interpersonal  relations  can  trace  their  sources. 

Roughly  contemporaneous  with  Mead  was  another  great  pioneer  in 
the  field  of  culture  and  personality — Edward  Sapir.  Working  as  a 
linguist-anthropologist,  Professor  Sapir  directly  inspired  many  of  the 
most  well-known  theories  and  investigations  in  the  field  of  cultural 
anthropology.  As  early  as  the  year  1927,  Sapir  was  stressing  the  im- 
portance of  interpersonal  communication: 

If  one  is  at  all  given  to  analysis,  one  is  impressed  with  the  extreme  complexity 
of  the  various  types  of  human  behavior,  and  it  may  be  assumed  that  the  things 
we  take  for  granted  in  our  ordinary,  everyday  life  are  as  strange  and  as  un- 
explainable  as  anything  we  might  find.  Thus,  one  comes  to  think  that  the 
matter  of  speech  is  very  far  from  being  the  self-evident  or  simple  thing  that  we 
think  it  to  be;  that  it  is  capable  of  a  very  great  deal  of  refined  analysis  from  the 
standpoint  of  human  behavior;  and  that  one  might,  in  the  process  of  making 
such  analyses,  accumulate  certain  ideas  for  the  research  of  personality  problems. 
There  is  one  thing  that  strikes  us  as  interesting  about  speech;  on  the  one 
hand,  we  find  it  difficult  to  analyze;  on  the  other  hand,  we  are  very  much 
guided  by  it  in  our  actual  experience.  That  is,  perhaps,  something  of  a  paradox, 
yet  both  the  simple  mind  and  the  keenest  of  scientists  know  very  well  that  we 
do  not  react  to  the  suggestions  of  the  environment  in  accordance  with  our 
specific  knowledge  alone.  Some  of  us  are  more  intuitive  than  others,  it  is  true, 
but  none  is  entirely  lacking  in  the  ability  to  gather  and  be  guided  by  speech 
impressions  in  the  intuitive  exploration  of  personality.  We  are  taught  that 
when  a  man  speaks  he  says  something  that  he  means  to  communicate.  That,  of 
course,  is  not  necessarily  so.  He  intends  to  say  something,  as  a  rule,  yet  what 
he  actually  communicates  may  be  measurably  different  from  what  he  started 
out  to  convey.  We  often  form  a  judgment  of  what  he  is  by  what  he  does  not 
say,  and  we  may  be  very  wise  to  refuse  to  limit  the  evidence  for  judgment  to 
the  overt  content  of  speech.   (9,  pp.  892-93 ) 

Later  in  the  same  paper  Sapir  summarizes:  "It  should  be  fairly  clear 
from  our  hasty  review  that  if  we  make  a  level-to-level  analysis  of  the 
speech  of  an  individual  and  if  we  carefully  see  each  of  these  levels  in 


I02  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

its  social  perspective,  we  obtain  a  valuable  lever  for  psychiatric  work. 
It  is  possible  that  the  kind  of  analysis  vi^hich  has  here  been  suggested, 
if  carried  far  enough,  may  enable  us  to  arrive  at  certain  very  pertinent 
conclusions  regarding  personality."  (9,  p.  905) 

If  these  predictions  made  over  a  quarter  of  a  century  ago  seem  most 
in  tune  with  current  trends,  it  can  hardly  be  considered  accidental. 
Working  with  Sapir  at  the  time  were  many  theorists  who  have  since 
become  well-known  exponents  of  the  culture  theory  of  personality — 
Sullivan,  Dollard,  Thomas. 

The  level  of  behavior  which  is  operationally  defined  in  this  chapter 
as  the  Level  of  Public  Communication  possesses,  therefore,  a  most 
eminent  scientific  heritage.  Starting  from  Darwin  and  Wundt's  con- 
cern with  the  gestural  expression  of  emotion,  taking  its  philosophic 
roots  in  the  linguistic  concepts  of  Sapir  and  Mead,  and  finding  its 
psychiatric  application  in  the  writings  of  Fromm,  Homey,  Moreno, 
and  Sullivan,  the  basic  notion  of  interpersonal  communication  has  for 
a  century  excited  the  interest  of  socially  oriented  theorists. 

The  first  clinical  and  empirical  approach  to  interpersonal  communi- 
cation was  developed  by  J.  L.  Moreno.  Many  ingenious  and  creative 
innovations  were  introduced  by  this  pioneering  worker.  For  over 
twenty  years  Moreno  has  employed  sociometric  methods  to  study 
group  structure.  These  techniques  indicate  the  bonds  of  attraction 
and  repulsion  which  exist  among  group  members  and  provide  an 
objective  picture  of  the  pattern  of  interpersonal  relationships. 
Moreno's  valuable  contributions  have  not  been  fully  exploited  because 
of  the  absence  of  empirical  studies.  Moreno  has  not  based  his  meas- 
urements upon  a  system  of  interpersonal  variables.  His  sociometric 
methods  possess  considerable  functional  value,  but  they  do  not  pro- 
vide an  interpersonal  diagnosis  in  terms  of  a  fixed  system  of  variables. 

In  the  last  five  years  three  comprehensive  empirical  systems  for 
classifying  interpersonal  behavior  have  been  described  in  the  litera- 
ture. Bales  ( 1 )  has  presented  a  reliable  and  effective  method  of  cate- 
gorizing interpersonal  processes  in  terms  of  positive,  negative,  or  neu- 
tral orientation  toward  a  group  goal.  This  has  been  applied  mainly 
to  group  decisions  and  group  problem-solving  behavior.  The  English 
psychiatrist  Bion  (2)  and  his  American  follower  Thelen  (11)  have 
developed  a  method  of  rating  the  individual's  response  to  the  group 
experience.  This  has  been  applied  to  problems  of  social  structure  in 
psychotherapy  groups  and  to  group-dynamics  situations.  The  third 
method  for  measuring  social  interaction  is  the  interpersonal  system  de- 
scribed in  this  book.  The  systems  developed  by  Bales  and  Bion  are 
major  methodological  achievements.  They  differ  from  the  present 
interpersonal  system  in  several  respects.  They  are  not  tied  to  a  theory 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


[03 


of  personality  nor  a  system  of  multilevel  measurement.  The  aim  of 
Bales  and  Bion  is  to  classify  behavior  that  is  most  crucial  to  their  par- 
ticular goals — group  problem  solving  and  group  therapy  process.  The 
aim  of  the  interpersonal  system  is  to  develop  a  method  of  measuring 
interpersonal  behavior  which  will  be  coordinate  with  the  measures  of 
interpersonal  behavior  at  other  levels  of  personality  and  which  will  fit 
into  a  multilevel  pattern  of  interpersonal  diagnosis.  The  reflexes  of 
Level  I  are,  perhaps,  the  most  crucial  aspect  of  personality,  but  from 
the  standpoint  of  functional  diagnosis  and  dynamic  theory  design  they 
must  fit  into  a  multilevel  structure. 

In  selecting  the  variables  for  classifying  Level  I  communication,  we 
have  kept  in  mind,  therefore,  not  the  purpose  or  structure  or  task  of 
the  group,  but  the  structure  of  the  individual's  total  personality.  With 
this  discussion  as  background,  we  shall  now  consider  the  empirical 
methodology  developed  for  classifying  interpersonal  behavior. 

Listing  the  Interpersonal  Reflexes.  To  make  objective  meas- 
urements of  the  reflex  phenomena  of  Level  I,  it  was  necessary  to  have 
a  finite  and  defined  list  of  interpersonal  behaviors.  Such  a  matrix  has 
been  presented  in  the  form  of  the  sixteen-point  circular  continuum. 
The  problem  becomes  that  of  determining  the  various  interactions 
which  reflect  the  sixteen  basic  motivations.  Because  we  are  dealing  at 
this  level  of  personality  with  communication  process — what  one  per- 
son does  to  another — it  is  convenient  to  use  verbs  (transitive  verbs) 
as  the  descriptive  terms.  Figure  1,  Chapter  6,  presents  the  sixteen 
generic  interpersonal  themes  along  with  a  list  of  sample  activities 
which  illustrates  the  range  of  each  point  around  the  circle. 

For  each  generic  theme  there  is,  of  course,  an  inexhaustible  list  of 
verbs.  The  terms  used  here  are  most  appropriate  for  verbal  exchanges 
in  therapeutic  or  diagnostic  contexts.  Thus,  we  suggest  that  to  boast, 
to  act  narcissistic  ally,  to  establish  autonomy  and  independence,  to  act 
self-confident  all  contain  about  the  same  proportion  of  dominance- 
hostility  as  indicated  by  the  point  B  on  the  circle.  By  this  we  mean 
that  they  express  the  same  qualitative  purpose  of  narcissistic  self-ap- 
proval. The  fact  that  they  difl^er  in  amount,  degree,  or  extremity  of 
the  purpose  is  handled  by  the  intensity  scale.  Other  lists  are  necessary 
for  categorizing  nonverbal  actions  (frowns,  gestures,  voice  tones)  and 
preverbal  situations  (nursery  school  interactions,  etc.). 

To  illustrate  this  system  of  scoring  social  behavior,  two  samples  of 
interaction  are  here  presented:  a  section  of  a  modern  play,  and  a  non- 
verbal nursery  school  exchange.  A  detailed  description  and  illustration 
of  the  use  of  several  Level  I  measures  in  the  psychiatric  clinic  will  be 
found  in  Appendix  L 


I04 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Mechanisms  as  Applied  to  a 
Conversation  in  a  Modern  Play.  The  following  passage  represents 
a  conversation  among  three  of  the  central  characters  of  Death  of  a 
Salesman  by  Arthur  Miller.^  The  scoring  of  each  interpersonal  reflex 
involves  three  items:  the  code  letter  representing  the  location  of  the 
action  along  the  circular  continuum,  the  verb  considered  most  closely 
descriptive  of  the  action,  and  the  rating  of  intensity  of  the  mechanism 
along  the  4-point  scale.  The  exchange  between  Biff  and  his  mother, 
Linda,  serves  as  a  nice  illustration  of  a  punitive-guilty  relationship. 


Linda:  You're  a  pair  of  animals!  Not  one,  not 
another  living  soul  would  have  had  the 
cruelty  to  w^alk  out  on  that  man  in  a 
restaurant. 

Biff,  not  looking  at  her:  Is  that  what  he  said? 

Linda:  He  didn't  have  to  say  anything.  He  was 
so  humiliated  he  nearly  limped  when  he 
came  in. 

Happy:  But,  Mom,  he  had  a  great  time  with  us. 

Biff,  cutting  him  off  violently:  Shut  up. 

[Without  another  word,  Happy  goes  upstairs.] 

Linda:  You!  You  didn't  even  go  to  see  if  he 
was  all  right! 

Biff,  sail  on  the  floor  in  front  of  Linda,  the 
flowers  in  his  hand;  with  self-loathing:  No, 
Didn't.  Didn't  do  a  damned  thing.  How  do 
you  like  that,  heh?  Left  him  babbhng  in  a 
toilet. 

Linda:  You  louse.   You. 

Biff:  Now  you  hit  it  on  the  nose!  [He  gets  up, 
throws  flowers  in  the  wastebasket.]  The 
scum  of  the  eanh,  and  you're  looking  at 
him! 

Linda:  Get  out  of  here! 


Inter- 
personal 
mechanism 

Descrip- 
tive verb 

Inten- 
sity 

E 

Condemn 

3 

J 

Inquire 

1 

P 

Inform 

1 

L 
B 

Conciliate 
Resist 

2 
1 

D 

Coerce 

3 

H 

Withdraw 

3 

F 

Complain 

2 

H 

Condemn  self 

3 

Condemn  3 

Condemn  self  3 


Coerce 


The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Mechanisms  as  Applied  to  Non- 
verbal Interaction  in  a  Nursery  School  Situation.  The  next  ex- 
ample of  the  scoring  of  interpersonal  reflexes  presents  qualitative  de- 
scriptions of  the  behavior  of  three  children  in  a  nonverbal  nursery 

^  Arthur  Miller,  Death  of  a  Salesman  (New  York:  Viking  Press,  1949),  pp.  124- 
125;  quoted  by  permission  of  the  publisher. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


105 


school  situation.   The  reflexes  are  scored  in  the  same  fashion  as  the 
verbal  interchanges  described  above. 


[Child  A  Is  playing  with  a  drum.] 

1.  Child  B     Runs  up   and   tries   to   pull   drum 

away. 

2.  Child  A    Tries  to  run  away. 

3.  Child  B    Trips  A 

and  pulls  drum  away. 

4.  Child  A    Stays  on  ground, 

sobs  loudly. 

5.  Child  B     Parades  with  drum,  pounding  it  in 

exhibitionistic  manner. 

6.  Child  C    Enters  play  area,  walks  to  Child  A. 

7.  Child  A    Cries  louder  and  pushes  Child  C 

away  defensively. 

8.  Child  C    Pushes  Child  A  back  and  throws 

dirt  on  him. 

9.  Child  B     Puts  drum  down  and  throws  dirt 

on  Child  A. 


Inter- 
personal 
mechanism 

Descrip- 
tive verb 

Inten- 
sity 

c 

Takes  by  force 

H 

Withdraws 

E 

c 

Attacks 
Takes  by  force 

H 
F 

Withdraws 
Complains 

B 

Exhibits 
superiority 

N 

Comforts 

F 

Complains 

Attacks 


Attacks 


This  interaction  exemplifies  the  way  in  which  individuals  train 
others  to  reject  and  attack  them.  Notice  how  skillfully  Child  A  (in- 
teractions 6,  7,  and  8)  pulls  aggressive  behavior  from  an  initially  well- 
intentioned  sympathizer  as  well  as  from  his  original  tormentor. 

As  we  observe  from  this  last  nonverbal  interaction,  the  judgment  of 
interpersonal  reflexes  is  quite  independent  of  the  concrete  medium  of 
their  expression.  The  mechanism  of  sympathize  is  scored  for  a  non- 
verbal pat  on  the  back  as  well  as  for  a  solely  verbal  reassurance — or 
from  a  combination  of  both. 

Five  Methods  for  Measuring  Level  I  Interpersonal  Reflexes. 
In  the  preceding  chapter  it  was  pointed  out  that  there  are  several  meth- 
ods for  obtaining  Level  I  public  communications.  To  insure  clarity 
we  have  established  the  working  rule  that  any  mention  of  Level  I  be- 
havior must  include  a  reference  to  the  specific  source  of  the  data. 

When  minute-by-minute  ratings  are  made  by  psychologists  of  in- 
terpersonal behavior  (either  observed  directly  or  derived  from  record- 
ings and  transcriptions)  the  resulting  data  are  assigned  to  Level  1-R. 

MMPI  indices  which  reflect  the  interpersonal  pressure  generated 
by  the  patient's  symptoms  are  coded  Level  I-M. 


io5  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

MMPI  indices  which  predict  future  interpersonal  behavior  in  group 
psychotherapy  are  coded  Level  I-P. 

Scores  from  standardized  situation  tests  which  summarize  the  sub- 
ject's reactions  are  coded  Level  1-T.^ 

When  the  subject's  interpersonal  role  is  summarized  on  the  Inter- 
personal Check  List  by  observers  or  fellow  patients  who  have  been  in- 
teracting with  him,  the  resulting  sociometric  indices  are  coded  Level 
IS. 

The  Level  I-R  ratings  of  interpersonal  reflexes  are  the  basic  meas- 
ures considered  in  the  theoretical  discussions  of  this  chapter.  In  rou- 
tine clinical  practice,  however,  we  have  found  it  necessary  to  rely  on 
two  sets  of  MMPI  indices  of  Level  I  behavior.  The  Level  I  diagnosis 
discussed  in  the  clinical  chapters  of  this  book  is  based  on  MMPI  meas- 
ures of  symptomatic  behavior  {Level  I-M).  The  predictions  of  be- 
havior in  group  psychotherapy,  derived  from  the  MMPI,  are  labeled 
Level  I-P. 

The  reasons  for  employing  these  MMPI  indices  will  now  be  dis- 
cussed. 

Level  I-M  Estimates  of  Symptomatic  Behavior.  Level  I-R  or 
Level  I-S  measurements  are  obtained  from  ratings  of  the  subject's  be- 
havior by  others  who  have  been  interacting  with  him  or  observing  his 
interactions.  It  is,  by  definition,  necessary  that  the  subject  be  involved 
in  social  relationships  in  order  to  make  the  Level  I-R  and  Level  I-S 
judgments.  The  ideal  source  of  these  ratings  is  the  group  therapy  sit- 
uation where  the  subject's  impact  on  several  others  can  be  determined. 

This  poses  a  practical  problem,  however.  The  functional  system  of 
personality,  which  we  are  presenting  in  this  book,  is  anchored  to 
Level  I-R  and  Level  I-S.  Since  this  is  the  "action  level,"  we  consider 
it  to  be  the  level  of  greatest  immediate  importance.  In  accomplishing 
interpersonal  diagnosis'*  at  the  time  of  intake  into  the  psychiatric 
clinic.  Level  I-R  and  I-S  ratings  are  generally  not  available.  There  has 
usually  been  no  opportunity  to  observe  the  patient  in  extended  inter- 
actions. In  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Clinic,  the  tests  are  generally  ad- 
ministered after  one  intake  interview  with  a  clinician.    For  many 

^  The  use  of  a  situation  test  will  not  be  illustrated  in  this  chapter.  The  Kaiser 
Foundation  project  is  now  engaged  in  developing  a  standardized  set  of  items  for  such 
a  test.  The  test  is  being  developed  as  a  multiple-choice  instrument  and  the  patient's 
responses  will  be  summarized  and  plotted  in  the  same  way  as  the  other  interpersonal 
scores. 

*  The  system  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  is  described  in  Chapter  12  of  this  book. 
Functional  diagnosis  is  based  on  the  multilevel  interpersonal  diagnosis  and  upon  the 
diagnosis  of  variability  (i.e.,  interlevel  conflict).  The  use  of  the  functional  system  of 
personality  in  accomplishing  clinical  diagnosis  and  prognosis  is  described  in  Chapters 
IS  through  22. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


107 


reasons  it  is  impossible  to  get  reliable  Level  I-R  and  I-S  ratings  from 
intake  workers  after  one  hour  of  interviewing.  Thus,  at  the  time  the 
functional  diagnostic  system  is  called  upon  to  make  its  predictions,  it 
is  forced  to  operate  without  its  most  important  level  of  personality — 
the  level  of  public  communication. 

The  ideal  solution  to  this  problem  would  be  to  develop  methods  for 
obtaining  reliable  estimates  of  Level  I-S  and  I-R  from  the  patient's 
interpersonal  behavior  at  the  time  of  intake  interview  and  testing. 
Several  factors — systematic  and  administrative — have  made  this  pro- 
cedure infeasible.  The  problem  of  including  estimates  of  the  patient's 
purposive  interpersonal  impact  at  the  time  of  intake  diagnosis,  as  ex- 
pressed through  his  symptoms,  has  been  met  by  developing  psycho- 
metric indices. 

Every  psychological  symptom  seems  to  have  an  interpersonal  mean- 
ing, i.e.,  impHcations  as  to  what  the  patient  is  communicating  through 
the  symptom,  and  what  the  patient  expects  to  be  done  about  it,  etc. 
Symptoms  are  usually  the  overt  reason  for  the  patient  coming  to  the 
clinic;  they  express  an  interpersonal  message. 

In  order  to  measure  the  symptomatic  impact  of  the  patient  upon 
the  clinician,  we  have  combined  eight  MMPI  scales  into  indices  which 
can  be  plotted  on  the  circular  diagnostic  grid.  The  Level  I  diagnoses 
employed  in  the  research  studies  described  in  this  book  are  based  on 
these  symptomatic  indices.  These  measures  are  coded  Level  I-M.  The 
MMPI  formulas  used  to  derive  these  indices  are:  vertical  (dominance- 
submission)  index  =  Ma  -}-  Hs  —  D  —  Ft;  the  horizontal  (love-hate) 
index  =  Hy  +  K  —  F  —  Sc.  These  MMPI  scale  abbreviations  and 
the  methodology  for  measuring  Level  I-M  are  described  in  Appen- 
dix 1. 

These  indices  have  considerable  functional  value  for  two  reasons. 
First,  they  indicate  the  interpersonal  reflexes  employed  by  the  pa- 
tient in  approaching  the  clinic  (as  indicated  by  his  motivation  and  the 
symptomatic  pressure  he  exerts).  In  the  clinical  situation  where  a  de- 
cision as  to  treatment  and  prognosis  is  the  main  functional  issue,  the 
interpersonal  messages  picked  up  by  the  MMPI  predictive  indices  are 
exactly  what  the  diagnostician  must  sense.  The  second  value  of  these 
indices  is  that  they  are  based  on  routine  test  procedures  which  are 
given  at  the  time  of  intake  evaluation.  They  provide  necessary  esti- 
mates of  Level  I-S  behavior  which  would  otherwise  be  lacking.  They 
plug  up  a  most  crucial  gap  in  the  multilevel  diagnostic  pattern. 

The  formulas  for  converting  MMPI  profiles  into  interpersonal 
measures  are  presented  in  Appendix  1 .  The  norms  for  converting  the 
Level  I-M  dominance  and  love  indices  into  standard  scores  are  pre- 
sented in  Appendix  5. 


,o8  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

Level  I-P  Indices  for  Predicting  Interpersonal  Behavior  in 
Group  Psychotherapy.  In  selecting  patients  for  therapy  groups  and 
in  planning  the  course  of  individual  therapy,  it  is  obviously  helpful  to 
have  a  foreknowledge  of  the  expected  interpersonal  behavior.  The 
Level  I-M  indices  predict  fairly  well  future  behavior  in  group  therapy. 
We  have  run  several  studies  in  which  Level  I-M  ratings  were  corre- 
lated with  sociometric  ratings  of  Level  I-S.  These  results  are  con- 
tained in  another  publication.  (6) 

There  were  many  cases,  however,  in  which  the  Level  I-M  did  not 
predict  actual  behavior  in  the  group.  The  inaccurate  forecasts  were 
due  to  the  fact  that  the  group  situation  can  pull  responses  from  the 
patient  that  are  different  from  his  symptomatic  behavior.  A  patient 
who  is  depressed  and  puts  dependent  pressure  on  the  intake  inter- 
viewer may  become  overconventional  or  helpful  in  his  reactions  to  the 
future  therapy  group  members.  A  different  sublevel  seems  to  be  in- 
volved. The  symptomatic  indices  seem  to  predict  individual  therapy 
(i.e.,  face-to-face  reactions  with  a  therapist)  better  than  group  ther- 
apy. This  means  that  we  can  take  an  MMPI  profile  and  calculate  the 
Level  I-M  scores  and  forecast  what  the  patient  is  going  to  do  to  the 
intake  worker  in  planning  treatment  or  to  a  future  individual  therapist. 
These  MMPI  cues  work  less  well  ifi  forecasting  what  the  patient  will 
do  to  other  patients  in  a  group. 

For  this  reason  a  set  of  indices  was  devised  which  specifically  pre- 
dicts behavior  in  group  therapy.  Two  special  MMPI  scales  for  pre- 
dicting dominant  or  hostile  roles  were  developed.  These  are  labeled 
Level  I-P. 

These  scales  were  based  on  item  analyses  which  studied  the  rela- 
tionship of  Each  MMPI  item  to  Level  I  Sociometric  indices  of  group 
patients.  The  MMPI  indices  which  predict  the  patient's  role  in  group 
therapy  make  it  possible  to  plot  on  the  diagnostic  grid  the  patient's 
Level  I-M  score.  This  predictive  index  is  useful  in  assigning  patients  to 
therapy  groups.  This  is  done  as  follows:  We  derive  the  Level  I-P  in- 
dices for  all  patients  on  the  group-therapy  waiting  list  and  plot  them 
on  the  same  diagnostic  grid.  It  is  then  possible  to  tell  at  a  glance  the 
range  of  role  behaviors  to  be  expected.  The  attempt  is  made  to  keep 
groups  heterogeneous  in  respect  to  roles.  That  is,  we  do  not  want 
any  group  overloaded  with  one  interpersonal  type.  Experience  has 
demonstrated  that  a  group  comprised  of  hysterics  will  tend  to  manifest 
the  same  interpersonal  reflexes,  and  interaction  among  patients  is 
minimized.  Similarly,  a  group  with  several  passively  resistant  person- 
alities will  tend  to  bog  down  into  silence  and  mutual  distrust. 

Selection  of  group  patients  is  somewhat  like  the  casting  of  parts  in 
a  play.  We  encourage  lively  interchanges  among  group  members  in 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  109 

which  different  role  interactions  develop.  Predictive  indices  from 
MMPI  scales  which  forecast  expected  behavior  are  the  basis  for  as- 
signing patients  to  groups.  They  also  assist  the  therapist  by  alerting 
him  to  the  pressures  which  will  develop. 

The  use  of  MMPI  predictive  indices  is  illustrated  in  Appendix  1  and 
in  Chapter  26. 

Routine  Reflex  Patterns.  During  any  one  day  the  average  adult 
runs  into  a  wide  range  of  interpersonal  stimuli.  We  are  challenged, 
pleased,  bossed,  obeyed,  helped,  and  ignored  on  an  average  of  several 
times  a  day.  Thus,  the  person  whose  entire  range  of  interpersonal 
reflexes  is  functioning  flexibly  can  be  expected  to  demonstrate  ap- 
propriately each  of  the  sixteen  interpersonal  reflexes  many  times  in  any 
day. 

There  are,  however,  many  who  do  not  react  with  consistent  ap- 
propriateness or  flexibility.  One  might  respond  to  the  pleasant  as  well 
as  the  rude  stranger  with  a  disapproving  frown.  Another  might  smile 
in  a  friendly  fashion.  If  we  study  an  extended'  sample  of  a  subject's 
interactions,  an  interesting  fact  develops.  Each  person  shows  a  con- 
sistent preference  for  certain  interpersonal  reflexes.  Other  reflexes  are 
very  difficult  to  elicit  or  absent  entirely.  It  is  possible  to  predict  in 
probability  terms  the  preferred  reflexes  for  most  individuals  in  a  spe- 
cific situation.  A  small  percentage  of  individuals  exist  who  get  "others" 
to  react  to  them  in  the  widest  range  of  possible  behaviors  and  who 
can  utilize  a  wide  range  of  appropriate  reactions.  Most  individuals 
tend  to  train  "others"  to  react  to  them  within  a  narrowed  range  of 
behaviors,  and  in  turn  show  a  restricted  set  of  favored  reflexes.  Some 
persons  show  a  very  limited  repertoire  of  two  or  three  reflexes  and 
reciprocally  receive  an  increasingly  narrow  set  of  responses  from 
others. 

Definition  of  Interpersonal  Role.  Most  everyone  manifests 
certain  automatic  role  patterns  which  he  automatically  assumes  in  the 
presence  of  each  significant  "other"  in  his  life.  These  roles  are  prob- 
ability tendencies  to  express  certain  interpersonal  purposes  with  sig- 
nificantly higher  frequency.  The  individual  may  be  quite  unaware  of 
these  spontaneous  tendencies — to  complain  to  his  wife,  to  be  stern 
with  his  children,  to  boss  his  secretary,  to  depend  on  the  office  man- 
ager. It  must  be  remembered  that  we  are  talking  here  in  statistical 
probability  terms.  The  subject  may  have  thousands  of  interactive  ex- 
changes each  day  with  each  of  his  significant  "others,"  and  these  may 
range  all  over  the  interpersonal  continuum.  When  we  obtain  evi- 
dence that  he  consistently  and  routinely  tends  to  favor  certain  mecha- 
nisms with  one  individual  significantly  more  than  chance  and  tends  to 


,  ,o  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

pull  certain  responses  from  the  other  to  a  similar  degree,  then  a  role 
relationship  exists. 

This  selective  process  of  employing  a  narrowed  range  of  reflexes 
with  certain  "others"  works,  as  we  have  seen,  in  a  double  reinforcing 
manner.  Most  durable  relationships  tend  to  be  symbiotic.  Masochistic 
women  tend  to  marry  sadistic  men;  and  the  latter  tend  to  marry 
women  who  tend  to  provoke  hostility.  Dependent  men  tend  to  seek 
nurturant  superiors,  who  in  turn  are  most  secure  when  they  have 
docile  subordinates  to  protect. 

The  institutional  role  relationships,  boss-secretary,  prisoner-guard, 
student-teacher,  etc.,  tend  to  be  more  stereotyped  and  fixed.  Even 
so,  there  exists  some  room  for  role  variability.  Some  secretaries 
"mother,"  nag,  or  even  boss  their  nominal  superiors.  In  general,  how- 
ever, we  can  surmise  that  personality  factors  enter  into  the  choice  of 
occupation.  Those  people  who  are  least  anxious  and  most  secure  when 
they  are  submitting  to  and  depending  on  strong  authority  tend  to  seek 
and  hold  subordinate  jobs.  The  network  of  relationships  even  in  the 
simplest  office  setup  can  be  bewildering  in  its  multilevel  complexity. 
Even  so,  the  institutional  hierarchy  patterns  are  less  involved  than  the 
familial  relationships. 

How  A  Pessimistic  Man  Reproaches  His  Wife.  We  cite  here 
the  very  oversimplified  example  of  the  man  who  tends  to  complain  to 
his  wife.  By  this  we  mean  that  he  reacts  to  his  wife  with  the  reflex 
of  grumbling  reproach  (FG)  with  increased  frequency,  often  to  an 
inappropriate  extreme.  His  voice  may  take  on  a  tired,  whiny  quality 
the  minute  he  enters  the  house.  He  can  be,  and  often  is,  jolly,  firm,  or 
protective  with  his  spouse.  But  as  we  pile  up  the  thousands  of  inter- 
action ratings  the  trend  towards  mild  complaint  becomes  increasingly 
clear. 

Now  we  call  these  reactions  reflexes  because  they  are  not  deliberate 
or  planned.  He  does  not  deliberately  decide  to  inject  the  hurt,  tired 
note  in  his  voice.  He  does  not  plan  the  slight  droop  of  the  shoulders. 
He  may  not  be  aware  of  the  continuous  mild  passive  irritation. 

He  may  not  even  know  the  basic  or  broader  reason  for  his  bitter- 
ness. It  might  take  some  weeks  of  therapeutic  exploration  for  him  to 
verbalize  his  private  feelings:  (1)  that  he  is  a  defeated  genius  whose 
failure  was  caused  by  his  wife,  (2)  that  he  could  be  a  success  today  if 
she  had  not  persuaded  him  to  marry  and  leave  engineering  school,  ( 3 ) 
that  he  might  be  a  rich  man  today  if  she  had  not  persuaded  him  to  give 
up  that  off^er  in  Texas,  etc.  More  intensive  analysis  would,  of  course, 
allow  the  roots  of  these  feelings  to  be  traced  back  even  further  in  the 
patient's  history. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  m 

Now  this  has  been  the  oversimpliiied  sketch  of  the  interpersonal 
reflex  repertoire  of  an  essentially  normal  man  and  an  essentially  normal 
marriage.  He  is  within  normal  limits  because  he  maintains  a  reason- 
ably flexible  range  of  interpersonal  behavior.  He  probably  can  em- 
ploy the  entire  continuum  of  reflexes  when  appropriate,  and  without 
anxiety.  If  we  could  sum  up  all  his  interactions  with  all  others  over 
a  period  of  time,  we  would  see  that  all  sixteen  reflexes  have  been 
elicited,  but  that  he  tends  to  favor  or  overem.phasize  the  mechanisms  of 
passive  complaint  and  distrustful,  realistic  hesitancy.  He  can  lead,  he 
can  express  independence,  he  can  support  others,  but  he  tends  to  a 
moderate  but  significant  degree  to  favor  a  grumpy  bitterness  in  his 
dealing  with  others. 

How  He  Provokes  Superior  Scorn  from  Four  Strangers.  This 
man  entered  a  pilot  study  therapy  group  along  with  four  other  stran- 
gers. After  eight  sessions,  a  summary  was  made  of  his  interpersonal 
behavior  toward  the  other  four  group  members.  The  same  pattern 
was  revealed.  At  times  he  lectured,  argued,  helped,  cooperated,  but 
the  mechanisms  which  he  spontaneously  favored  and  manifested  a  sig- 
nificant majority  of  the  time  were  those  of  passive  resistance.  At  the 
same  time,  a  summary  was  made  of  the  interactions  this  man  pulled 
from  the  others  in  the  group,  i.e.,  what  they  did  to  him.  Again  a  fairly 
flexible  pattern  resulted.  They  listened  to  him  with  respect,  they  de- 
ferred to  him,  accepted  his  help,  but  the  most  frequent  purposive  be- 
havior directed  toward  him  was  a  mildly  critical  superiority  (BCD). 
They  liked  him,  respected  him,  but  on  the  whole  felt  moderate 
patronizing  scorn  in  reaction  to  his  grumbling  approach. 

Notice  that  in  seven  sessions  of  brief  interaction  this  subject  (like 
the  sample  case  described  earlier)  succeeded  in  duplicating  his  life 
situation  with  the  four  strangers  in  the  group.  This  man,  it  must  be 
remembered,  is  essentially  normal.  He  can  react  flexibly  and  ap- 
propriately to  most  interpersonal  situations.  He  has  his  favored  inter- 
personal techniques  for  handling  anxiety,  as  we  all  do,  and  this  mild 
imbalance  in  the  direction  of  grumpy  pessimism  gives  him  uniqueness 
and  identity  as  a  human  being.  His  wife  and  his  friends,  very  likely, 
understand  and  adapt  to  his  gloomy  realism  with  humorous  (and 
sometimes  irritable)  impatience. 

A  Masterful  Defense  of  Sullen  Distrust.  A  different  picture 
develops  when  we  turn  to  the  maladjustive  patterns.  The  suspicious, 
isolated,  immobilized  case  described  earlier  in  this  chapter  had  an  ex- 
tremely limited  repertoire  of  social  responses.  He  reacted  in  almost 
every  situation  with  resentful  distrust — to  the  group  members,  to  his 
parents,  to  his  acquaintances.  It  was  next  to  impossible  to  elicit  a  sym- 


112 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


pathetic  or  nurturant  response  from  this  patient.  These  interpersonal 
reflexes  were  completely  inhibited.  The  expressive  behavior  of  this 
man  was  saturated  with  sullen,  wary,  growling  distrust.  By  con- 
sistently exhibiting  this  narrow  range  of  behavior  in  situations  when 
they  may  or  may  not  have  been  appropriate,  he  had  trained  the  group 
members  in  the  same  manner  that  he  had  trained  all  the  others  in  his 
life  to  condemn  and  isolate  him.  This  patient's  reflexive  techniques  for 
provoking  rejection  were  so  well  developed  that  the  most  well-inten- 
tioned, friendly  approach  made  no  dent  in  his  armor.  However  sym- 
pathetic the  "other  one"  might  be,  his  masterful,  consistent  sullenness 
would  stimulate  eventual  impatience.  This  inevitable,  exasperated  re- 
jection, of  course,  increased  his  aggravation  and  would  tend  to  in- 
crease the  probability  of  further  isolation.  This  reciprocal  process  by 
which  human  beings  tend  to  pull  from  others  responses  that  tend  to 
maintain  their  limited  security  operations  will  be  discussed  under  the 
headings  of  'The  Principle  of  Self-Determination"  and  "The  Prin- 
ciple of  Reciprocal  Interpersonal  Relations."  Before  treating  these 
issues,  we  shall  pause  to  consider  some  of  the  methodological  impli- 
cations of  the  reflex  behavior  of  Level  I. 

It  will  be  recalled  that  the  Level  I  measure  is  always  in  terms  of  a 
rating  of  the  subject's  interpersonal  behavior  by  someone  else.  The 
subject  must  be  involved  in  a  social  situation.  The  Level  I  judgments 
are  then  made  by  the  observers — psychologists  or  fellow  participants 
in  the  interaction. 

Patients  as  Diagnostic  Instruments.  Our  measuring  instrument 
is,  therefore,  another  human  being.  Since  interpersonal  behavior  is  a 
functionally  important  dimension  of  personality,  it  is  quite  natural  that 
we  measure  it  directly — in  terms  of  the  actual  social  impact  that  the 
subject  has  on  others.  Some  interesting  implications  develop.  By  al- 
lowing the  patient  to  react  with  others — say  in  a  group  therapy  situa- 
tion— we  make  it  possible  for  him  to  demonstrate,  directly  and  openly, 
his  repertoire  of  interpersonal  reflexes.  He  tends  to  recreate  to  a  mild 
extent  in  the  group  his  neurotic  adjustment.  He  accomplishes  his 
own  interpersonal  diagnosis. 

The  therapeutic  group,  thus,  serves  as  a  small  subsociety,  a  minia- 
ture world.  The  members  of  a  therapy  group  have  a  valuable  diagnos- 
tic function.  When  we  ask  them  to  rate  each  other's  interpersonal  be- 
havior (on  a  check  list  or  sociometric  blank,  covering  the  range  of  the 
sixteen  generic  variables)  we  obtain  an  estimate  of  what  each  patient 
has  done  to  the  others. 

Why  Patients  Produce  Better  Interpersonal  Diagnoses  than 
Psychologists.    Patients  tend  to  rate  each  other  much  in  the  same 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


113 


way  as  trained  psychologists.  Sometimes  the  members  of  a  therapy 
group  see  a  fellow  patient  differently  from  the  therapist.  When  these 
discrepancies  in  Level  I  ratings  occur,  it  is  usually  the  psychiatrist  or 
psychologist  whose  judgments  are  less  accurate  and  less  valuable. 
Clinicians'  judgments  of  patients  tend  to  be  complicated  affairs.  They 
are  often  very  derived,  distorted  by  theoretical  or  "depth"  considera- 
tions. Naive,  untrained  subjects — fellow  patients,  family  members — 
generally  judge  each  other  in  terms  of  their  direct  reactions  to  the  sub- 
ject. They  tend  to  like,  fear,  respect  each  other,  and  their  ratings 
reflect  these  reactions.  They  do  not  "psychologize."  The  ratings  of 
trained  professional  workers  tend  to  be  much  more  intellectual.  It  is 
not  good  form  for  them  to  admit  that  they  like,  fear,  or  look  up  to  a 
patient.  Their  ratings,  indeed,  are  classically  supposed  to  be  divorced 
from  these  personal  reactions. 

We  have  found,  therefore,  that  psychologists  and  psychiatrists  tend 
to  give  interesting  and  theoretically  valid  ratings  of  deeper  motives  and 
future  developments.  Since  patients  spend  most  of  their  time  inter- 
acting with  nontrained,  psychologically  naive  individuals,  it  is  from 
the  latter  that  we  obtain  the  best  diagnosis  of  their  "main  street" 
stimulus  value.  The  meaning  of  such  a  rating  is  not  clear-cut.  Many 
extraneous  factors  influence  it — the  personality  of  the  rater  and  the 
climate  of  the  therapy  group,  to  name  two.  Some  of  these  complica- 
tions can  be  handled  by  standardizing  procedures  and  corrections  for 
perceptual  distortion  on  the  part  of  the  rater.  Others  cannot  be  con- 
trolled. Even  so,  the  rating  from  the  nonprofessional  is  much  more 
straightforward  an  estimate  of  Level  I  communication. 

Professional  Clinicians  as  Measuring  Instruments.  There  are 
very  good  reasons  why  trained  clinicians  are  less  effective  and  reliable 
when  asked  to  rate  single-level  variables.  The  factors  that  make  them 
good  clinicians  tend  to  hamper  their  becoming  good  rating  instru- 
ments. The  key  to  clinical  skill  is  the  ability  to  make  multilevel  ob- 
servations and  to  synthesize  them.  The  good  psychiatrist  is  trained  to 
perceive  many  cues  from  many  levels  at  one  time  and  to  act  upon 
these  cues  effectively. 

He  is  able  to  predict  what  the  multilevel  behavior  of  the  patient 
will  be  in  the  long-range  future,  and  also  in  the  immediate  future. 
Thus  he  develops  the  intuitive  expectation  as  to  the  patient's  reac- 
tion to  an  interpretative  intervention  on  his  part.  He  is  able  to  grasp 
what  the  patient  may  be  communicating  at  several  levels  as  he  free- 
associates.  He  attends,  now  to  the  verbal  content,  now  to  the  sym- 
bolic cues  (slips  of  the  tongue,  etc.),  now  to  the  immediate  inter- 
personal pressure  from  the  patient.   His  behavior  in  response  to  the 


1 14  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

patient's  activity  is  usually  based  on  a  complex  integration  of  these 
many  cues.  In  many  cases  he  does  not  stop  to  sort  out  all  these  cues 
into  categories.  He  may  be  hard  put  to  explain  exactly  what  it  was 
in  the  pattern  of  the  patient's  communication  that  led  him  to  his  con- 
clusion. 

Now  this  is  a  most  frustrating  situation  to  the  scientist  who  seeks 
to  measure  factors  involved  in  therapeutic  communication  or  diag- 
nosis. Scientific  ratings  are  supposed  to  be  reliable,  repeatable,  ex- 
plicit, specific.  At  this  primitive  stage  of  the  science  they  are  in- 
evitably single-level  measurements.  Often  the  scientist  is  very  dis- 
couraged when  he  attempts  to  pin  the  clinician  down.  He  knows  that 
the  clinician  responds  to  cues  with  considerable  skill.  But  his  rating 
scales  fail  miserably  to  tap  the  richness  of  the  clinical  experience.  The 
practitioner  is  likewise  frustrated  and  sometimes  irritated  by  what 
he  calls  the  "simple-minded"  quantitative  approach  of  the  scientist. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  project  has  worked  out  a  tentative  solu- 
tion to  this  dilemma  by  avoiding  the  use  of  clinicians  as  rating  instru- 
ments. The  clinician's  insights  are  employed  in  setting  up  the  system, 
in  determining  the  rating  categories.  Thus  we  have  long  discussions 
with  psychiatrists  and  practitioners  before  deciding  how  many  levels 
to  employ  in  the  system.  Clinicians  tell  us  what  kind  of  conflicts,  iden- 
tification patterns,  and  therapeutic  phenomena  they  run  into.  They 
produce  many  multilevel  hypotheses  about  personality  dynamics,  the 
nature  of  change  in  therapy,  and  so  on.  The  system  is  then  expanded 
and  revised  to  get  at  these  phenomena.  New  experimental  uses  for  the 
system  are  suggested.  A  clinician  may  report  that  certain  patients 
show  a  particular  imagery  of  "Father."  The  system  is  then  broadened 
to  get  at  this  dimension — we  may  add  a  new  rating  category  to  the 
TAT  analysis  which  picks  up  "symbolic  view  of  Father."  Clinical 
intuition  is  thus  mainly  responsible  for  what  kind  of  a  system  and  what 
kind  of  measurements  are  made. 

But  we  attempt  to  keep  the  clinician  far  away  from  the  actual 
measurement  process.  He  tells  us  what  to  measure;  but  we  do  not  ask 
him  to  measure  it  for  us.  At  the  present  time  the  project's  measure- 
ment procedures  (both  research  and  diagnostic)  are  executed  com- 
pletely by  nonprofessional  workers.  The  tests  are  administered  by 
trained  technicians.  The  specific  ratings  are  done  by  the  patient  about 
himself,  by  his  fellow  patient,  or  by  technicians  carefully  trained  to 
make  unilevel  judgments.  The  TAT  stories  are  not  employed  as  global 
productions  upon  which  multilevel  analyses  of  the  patient's  personality 
can  be  based.  They  are  defined  as  Level  III  data.  A  crew  of  intelli- 
gent, but  nonpsychologically  trained  technicians  then  moves  in  to  rate 
the  TAT  stories  for  the  interpersonal  themes.  These  technicians  are 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  115 

not  encumbered  with  the  complex  cHnical  skills  or  broad  theoretical 
conceptions.  Their  job  is  to  do  unilevel  ratings,  which  they  accom- 
plish with  straightforward  competence. 

The  standard  interpersonal  system  test  battery  includes  seven  dif- 
ferent tests.  These  are  administered,  scored,  rated,  and  profiled  by 
nonprofessional  or  semiprofessional  help.  In  essence,  the  hundreds  of 
molecular  scores  are  fed  into  the  system  and  the  resulting  matrix  of 
multidimensional  scores  is  handed  to  the  clinician  who  then  interprets 
it.  The  clinician  applies  his  creative,  intuitive  skills  to  understand  the 
complex  patterns  of  scores  and  to  relate  this  to  the  facts  that  he  has 
about  the  patient  from  the  interview.  He  makes  sense  out  of  a  pattern 
of  scores — a  task  which  neither  the  unsophisticated  patient  nor  the 
psychologically  untrained  technician  can  hope  to  perform.  The  pro- 
fessional energy  is  thus  applied  to  developing  the  machinery  of  the  sys- 
tem and  to  the  final  product  which  comes  from  this  machinery.  The 
running  of  the  machine  and  the  processing  of  the  measurements  (in- 
cluding ratings  of  symbolism)  are  accomplished  by  specially  trained 
technicians. 

There  is  one  occasion  upon  which  we  ask  clinicians  to  make  ratings. 
This  is  done  when  we  want  to  study  the  clinicians  and  not  the  patients. 
If  we  ask  twenty  clinical  workers  to  rate  a  group  of  patients  or  a  set 
of  test  scores  on  the  variable  repression  or  ego  strength,  the  results  tell 
us  how  the  individual  clinician  or  how  the  entire  group  of  clinicians 
conceive  of  these  two  variables.  In  two  research  explorations  done  by 
the  research  group  it  was  determined  that  the  clinical  psychologists 
who  rated  repression  and  ego  strength  relied  mainly  on  Level  II  cues. 
Patients  who  claimed  to  be  strong,  friendly,  and  healthy  were  rated 
as  repressers  and  having  strong  egos.  The  results  of  the  ratings  thus 
told  us  how  these  psychologists  conceived  of  the  variables  and  did  not 
necessarily  measure  the  variables  in  an  independently  vaUd  manner. 

When  we  ask  untrained  people  for  unilevel  ratings,  we  have  a  fair 
idea  of  the  meaning  of  the  data.  When  we  ask  clinicians  to  make  uni- 
level ratings,  we  are  misusing  their  complex  skills,  confusing  the  mean- 
ing of  the  system,  and  in  most  cases  lowering  reliability. 

The  Principle  of  Self -Determination 

In  the  preceding  pages  as  the  illustrative  case  material  has  unfolded, 
I  have  consistently  employed  a  rather  cumbersome  circumlocution  to 
describe  the  interaction  between  the  sample  subject  and  the  "others" 
with  whom  they  interact.  Most  statements  describing  what  "others" 
did  to  the  sample  case  were  worded  so  as  to  give  responsibihty  to  the 
subject.  Thus  we  say,  "He  trained  or  provoked  the  group  members 
to  reject  him,"  rather  than  "They  rejected  him."  In  the  listing  of  il- 


ii6  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

lustrative  interpersonal  reflexes  (Figure  i),  it  may  have  been  noted 
that  both  active  and  passive  phrases  were  used.  Thus  for  the  inter- 
personal reflex  G  we  have  included  acts  rejected  and  provokes  rejec- 
tion. We  take  the  subject  as  the  focus  of  attention  and  as  the  locus  of 
responsibility. 

I  have  tried  to  stress  the  surprising  ease  and  facility  with  which 
human  beings  can  get  others  to  respond  in  a  uniform  and  repetitive 
way.  Interpersonal  reflexes  operate  with  involuntary  routine  and 
amazing  power  and  speed.  Many  subjects  with  maladaptive  inter- 
personal patterns  can  provoke  the  expected  response  from  a  complete 
stranger  in  a  matter  of  minutes.  The  defiant  chip-on-the-shoulder; 
docile,  fawning  passivity;  timid,  anxious  withdrawal — these  are  some 
of  the  interpersonal  techniques  which  can  pull  the  reciprocal  reaction 
from  the  "other  one"  with  unfailing  regularity.  Severe  neurotics — de- 
fined at  this  level  as  individuals  with  limited  ranges  of  reflexes — are 
incredibly  and  creatively  skilled  in  drawing  rejection,  nurturance,  etc., 
from  the  people  with  whom  they  deal.  In  many  cases  the  "sicker"  the 
patient,  the  more  likely  he  is  to  have  abandoned  all  interpersonal  tech- 
niques except  one — which  he  can  handle  with  magnificent  finesse. 
Most  clinicians  who  have  dealt  with  the  disorder  will  be  glad  to  testify 
that  the  so-called  catatonic  negation  is  a  powerful  interpersonal  ma- 
neuver. 

Assigning  the  causative  factor  in  interpersonal  relations  to  the  sub- 
ject is  a  standard  procedure  in  dynamic  psychiatry.  The  skillful 
therapist  is  usually  not  inclined  to  join  the  abused,  unhappy,  masochis- 
tic patient  in  lamentation.  He  is  much  more  inclined  to  ask  himself 
and  eventually  the  patient,  "What  do  you  do  to  people  with  con- 
sistent and  consummate  skill  to  get  them  to  beat  you  up?"  The  prin- 
ciple involved  here  holds  that  interpersonal  events  just  do  not  happen 
to  human  beings  by  accident  or  external  design.  The  active  and  execu- 
tive role  is  given  to  the  subject. 

This  principle  (as  is  the  case  with  most  other  psychological  con- 
cepts) has  been  described  and  given  more  eloquent  expression  by 
novelists.  Here,  for  example,  is  D.  H.  Lawrence  outlining  the  notion 
of  self-determinism:  "No  man  .  .  .  cuts  another  man's  throat  unless 
he  wants  to  cut  it,  and  unless  the  other  man  wants  it  cut.  This  is  a 
complete  truth.  It  takes  two  people  to  make  a  murder:  a  murderer 
and  a  murderee.  And  a  murderee  is  a  man  who  is  murderable.  And 
who  is  murderable  is  a  man  who  in  a  profound  if  hidden  lust  desires 
to  be  murdered."  (5,  p.  36) 

Human  Beings  Resist  Taking  Responsibility  for  Their  Situa- 
tions.  This  point  of  view  plows  headlong  into  the  most  widespread 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  117 

resistance.  It  threatens  the  most  cherished  beliefs  of  Western  philos- 
ophy— from  Sophocles  (who  stresses  fate)  to  the  modern  mental 
hygienists  (who  overemphasize  parental  behavior) .  What  is  more  im- 
portant, it  threatens  the  most  cherished  illusions  of  the  average  man 
who  bases  his  security  and  self-esteem  on  the  traditional  procedure  of 
externalizing  blame. 

What  we  are  saying  here  to  the  human  being  is,  "You  are  mainly 
responsible  for  your  life  situation.  You  have  created  your  own  world. 
Your  own  interpersonal  behavior  has,  more  than  any  other  factor,  de- 
termined the  reception  you  get  from  others.  Your  slowly  developing 
pattern  of  reflexes  has  trained  others  and  yourself  to  accept  you  as 
this  sort  of  person — to  be  treated  in  this  sort  of  way.  You  are  the 
manager  of  your  own  destiny." 

This  attribution  of  responsibility  to  the  subject  we  have  called  the 
Principle  of  Self-Determination.  Although  it  has  the  deceptive  ap- 
pearance of  simplicity,  it  is,  on  the  contrary,  the  most  complex  kind  of 
concept.  To  this  notion  of  self-determination  the  average  person  is 
willing  to  give  halfhearted  and  halfway  approval.  It  is  easy  to  see 
where  the  successful  person  can  be  self-made.  He  chooses  his  goals, 
works  for  them,  and  makes  the  grade.  It  is  accepted  quite  naturally 
that  men  strive  and  bargain  for  the  interpersonal  goals  reflected  in  one 
half  of  the  spectrum — independence,  power,  popularity,  affection  (i.e., 
B,  A,  P,  O,  N,  M).  It  is  often  less  comprehensible  that  men  should 
actively  seek  the  interpersonal  states  represented  by  the  other  half  of 
the  circular  continuum — dependence,  weakness,  distrust,  and  self- 
effacing  modesty  (E,  F,  G,  H,  I,  J).  People,  it  is  held,  just  don't  seek 
to  defeat  themselves. 

How  Three  Human  Beings  Got  What  They  Bargained  for. 
A  patient  poignantly  reports:  "What  I  want  more  than  anything  else 
is  to  marry  a  dependent,  feminine  girl,  but  my  three  ex-wives  were 
bossy,  exploitive  tyrants."  This  man  may  at  the  level  of  conscious 
awareness  "want"  a  feminine  girl,  but  his  Level  I  behavior — im- 
mobilized, distrustful,  and  masochistic — is  enough  to  force  the  most 
neutral  woman  into  exasperated  activity. 

Another  patient  states:  "What  I  want  in  a  husband  is  a  strong,  suc- 
cessful man  who  will  take  care  of  me;  but  all  I  seem  to  attract  are 
penniless  artists  and  passive,  dreamy  bookworms."  This  woman  may, 
at  Level  II,  consciously  wish  for  a  strong  husband;  but  her  mothering, 
responsible  Level  I  reflexes  are  so  automatic  and  deeply  ingrained  that 
the  strongest  man  would  feel  smothered  and  alienated  by  the  ma- 
ternal stability,  to  which  dependent  men  are  drawn  with  moth-like 
fascination. 


ii8  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

A  third  patient  says:  "I  want  more  than  anything  else  to  finish  my 
college  training  and  get  established  in  a  profession,  but  all  the  profes- 
sors I  have  studied  with  are  narrow-minded  men  who  reject  my  ideas 
and  end  up  by  flunking  me."  This  man  may  wittingly  desire  the 
prestige  of  professorial  responsibility,  but  his  rebellious,  defiant  re- 
flexes eventually  exasperate  and  frustrate  even  the  most  sympathetic 
mentor.  What  human  beings  consciously  wish  is  often  quite  at  vari- 
ance with  the  results  that  their  reflex  patterns  automatically  create  for 
them.  For  these  people  the  sad  paradox  remains  that  voluntary  inten- 
tions, verbal  resolutions,  and  even  intellectual  insight  are  operationally 
feeble  and  numerically  infinitesimal  compared  to  the  ongoing  24-hour- 
a-day  activity  of  the  involuntary  interpersonal  reactions.  The  frus- 
trated student  just  mentioned  may  in  a  burst  of  intellectual  awareness 
decide  to  conform  to  the  academic  demands  and  return  to  college. 
This  resolution  is  a  conscious,  voluntary  efl"ort — very  much  like  prac- 
ticing for  an  hour  to  prevent  the  eye-wink  reflex  from  operating 
when  an  object  is  waved  in  front  of  it.  Such  conscious  control  cannot 
be  maintained  24  hours  a  day  or  he  would  be  able  to  concentrate  on 
nothing  but  the  eyelid  reflex.  Analogously,  our  rebellious  student 
faces  the  difficult  task  of  fighting  a  continuous,  exhausting  battle 
against  his  spontaneous  tendencies  (1)  to  sneer  and  balk  at  authori- 
ties, thus  (2)  pulling  from  them  an  eventual  impatient  rejection,  which 
(3)  increases  his  tendency  to  sneer  and  balk. 

Why  Human  Beings  Develop  the  Reflexes  of  Weakness  and 
Rejection.  A  second  logical  objection  to  the  concept  of  interpersonal 
reflex  looms  up  here.  The  eye-wink  reflex,  it  might  be  argued,  is 
naturally  acquired,  universal  to  all  men,  and  survivally  favorable.  The 
patterns  of  social  reactivity  cited  here  meet  none  of  these  criteria. 
What  is  the  rationale  which  explains  how  different  human  beings  de- 
velop different  rigid,  self-defeating  techniques  of  adjustment? 

The  first  point  to  note  is  that  we  are  concentrating  here — for  the 
sake  of  illustration — on  maladaptive  phenomena.  The  description  of 
these  extreme  reflexes  gone  wild  implies  that  other  reflexes  are,  in 
contrast,  inhibited.  The  masochistic  man  could  not  maintain  a  mini- 
mum of  independent  assertiveness;  the  maternal  role  prevented  the 
responsible  woman  from  manifesting  the  reflexes  of  docility  and  trust; 
the  rebel  possessed  conformity  and  affiliation  reflexes  which  were  qui- 
escent or  extinct. 

In  the  adjusted,  well-functioning  individual,  the  entire  repertoire 
of  interpersonal  reflexes  is  operating  spontaneously,  flexibly,  and  ap- 
propriately— and  when  the  survival  situation  demands  aggression,  he 
can  aggress;  when  it  calls  for  tenderness,  he  can  be  tender.   Human 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


19 


societies,  however,  tend  not  to  be  too  well  balanced.  They  tend  to 
put  a  premium  on  certain  interpersonal  responses — competitiveness  or 
slavish  submission,  for  example.  To  survive  and  flourish,  human  beings 
must  tailor  their  responses  to  the  demands  of  such  imbalanced  cultures. 
Even  in  the  most  heterogenous  and  tolerant  society  the  developing 
personality  interacts  with  so  many  inflexible  pressures  (e.g.,  parent's 
personalities,  subcultural  demands)  that  a  hierarchy  of  preferred  re- 
flexes develops.  To  say  that  human  personality  is  varied  and  different 
is  to  say — at  this  level — that  most  everyone  tends  to  overemphasize 
certain  automatic  interpersonal  responses  and  to  underemphasize 
others. 

The  questions  still  remain:  Why  do  human  beings  limit  their  ma- 
chinery of  social  adjustment,  manifest  narrowed  spectra  of  reaction, 
and  provoke  a  restricted  set  of  reactions  from  others?  Why  do  some 
individuals  have  no  ability  for  realistic,  modest  self-criticism  (H)  and 
compulsively  express  only  narcissistic  self-enhancing  mechanisms  (B)  ? 
Why  do  others  cling  to  retiring  modesty  and  eschew  the  responses  of 
proud  self-confidence?  Most  puzzling  of  all  (to  the  occidental  mind) : 
Why  do  some  of  our  neighbors  masochistically  court  interpersonal 
humiliation — doggedly  provoking  rejection  and  isolation  from  others? 

For  the  answers  to  these  questions  we  return  again  to  Sullivan.  He 
defines  personality  as  the  pattern  of  interpersonal  responses  employed 
to  reduce  anxiety,  ward  off  disapproval,  and  maintain  self-esteem.  As 
the  individual  develops,  he  discovers  that  certain  interpersonal  re- 
sponses bring  danger;  some  bring  a  narrow,  uncomfortable,  but  cer- 
tain security.  To  use  others  would  involve  broad,  attractive,  but 
conflictful  uncertainty.  The  more  anxiety-provoking  the  individual's 
world — particularly  his  parental  home — the  more  likely  he  is  to  select 
the  familiar,  narrow,  certain,  lesser  anxiety  and  to  avoid  the  promising 
but  uncertain  potentialities.  The  basic  meanings  behind  any  personal- 
ity pattern  are  difficult  to  evaluate.  The  complex  behavior  of  counter- 
poised motives  at  difl'erent  levels  creates  the  appearance  of  a  terribly 
anarchic  system.  It  is  clear,  however,  that  for  many  people  self- 
esteem  and  security  involve  surprising  maneuvers — including  extreme 
self-punishment  (at  Level  I).  The  "search  for  suffering"  (H)  can 
have  an  inexhaustible  number  of  meanings — all  functions  of  the  multi- 
level integration  of  personality.  Getting  her  husband  to  beat  and 
exploit  her  can  allow  the  masochist  externalization  of  guilt,  propitia- 
tion of  guilt,  passive  expression  of  hostility,  and  the  intense  pleasures 
of  narcissistic  martyred  self-pity.  "No  one  suffers  more  than  me; 
watch  and  I  can  prove  it."  The  projection  of  blame  for  failure  on 
others,  of  course,  requires  more  and  more  demonstration  of  failure 
in  order  to  maintain  the  allegation.  In  addition,  by  selecting  this  set  of 


120  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

aggression-provoking  responses,  the  masochist  avoids  the  potentially 
conflict-laden  area  of  active  hostility  (DE),  marital  collaboration  and 
-sexual  partnership  (LM),  and  assertive  responsibiUty  (BAP).  Most 
severe  martyrs,  of  course,  express  indirectly  the  purposive  behaviors 
that  they  inhibit  at  the  public  level.  They  accomplish  their  private 
aims — aggressive,  narcissistic,  exploitive,  and,  very  likely,  sexual — by 
the  indirect,  cumbersome,  and  unsatisfactory  method  of  acting  hurt 
and  provoking  aggression  from  others. 

The  Pressure  to  Repeat  Responses.  Interpersonal  activities  are 
designed  to  avoid  the  greater  anxiety.  It  might  be  said  in  general  that 
the  human  being  experiences  less  anxiety  in  a  familiar  situation  than 
in  a  strange  one,  and  less  anxiety  when  he  is  employing  familiar 
responses  than  strange  ones.  Reciprocal  relationships  with  crucial 
"others"  develop  quite  naturally  here.  The  more  an  individual  re- 
stricts his  actions  to  one  narrow  sector  of  the  interpersonal  spectrum, 
the  more  he  restricts  the  social  environment  he  faces.  That  is,  the  man 
who  continually  employs  submissive  reflexes  tends  to  train  people  to 
boss  him  and  discourage  people  from  looking  to  him  for  forceful 
leadership.  This  tendency  to  repeat  the  patterns  of  the  past  is  similar 
to  the  principal  of  least  action  which  is  described  by  Whitehead  (12, 
p.  108)  as  that  phenomenon  in  which  "cases  will  group  round  the  in- 
dividual perception  as  envisaging  (without  self-consciousness)  that 
one  immediate  possibility  of  attainment  which  represents  the  closest 
analogy  to  its  own  immediate  past."  The  interpersonal  world  of  the 
submissive  man  tends  to  become  quite  lopsided,  putting  more  and  more 
pressure  on  him  to  obey  and  not  to  command. 

Survival  anxiety  presses  the  individual  to  repeat  and  narrow  down 
his  adjustive  responses.  He  thus  comes  to  a  stable  but  restricted 
reciprocal  relationship  with  his  interpersonal  world.  But  this  is  only 
one  half  of  the  total  event. 

The  Pressure  to  Change  Responses.  In  addition  to  this  tendency 
for  the  familiar  personal  environment  to  become  limited  in  scope,  we 
have  seen  that  the  environment  at  large  presents  one  with  a  wide 
range  of  social  stimuh.  In  any  single  day  most  individuals  roaming 
around  in  their  ecological  space  find  suitable  situations  for  expressing 
all  sixteen  interpersonal  mechanisms.  To  the  extent  that  the  indi- 
vidual inhibits  some  of  these,  he  is  not  employing  the  appropriate 
responses  demanded  by  the  environment.  Failure  to  adapt  to  the  world 
about  it  generally  creates  survival  anxiety  in  the  organism. 

The  Insoluble  Dilemma  of  AdjustxMent:  Stability  Versus 
Flexibility.  The  human  being  is,  according  to  this  view,  caught  be- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  121 

tween  two  polar  whirlpools  of  anxiety.  Rigid  repetition  of  inter- 
personal responses  minimizes  conflict  and  provides  the  security  of  con- 
tinuity and  sameness — in  Whitehead's  useful  terminology  called 
"endurance."  But  the  environment  at  large  is  not  the  same — and  ad- 
justment to  it  demands  a  flexible  generality  of  inteqjersonal  response. 
The  notational  system  and  general  premises  of  the  present  work  lead 
us  to  conclude  that  this  is  the  critical  survival  dilemma — the  basic 
conflict,  if  you  please,  of  human  nature.  A  quotation  from  Egon 
Brunswik  (3)  appears  pertinent  here.  He  points  out  that  "survival  and 
its  sub-units,  which  may  be  defined  as  the  establishment  of  stable  inter- 
relationships with  the  environment,  are  possible  only  if  the  organism 
is  able  to  establish  compensatory  balance  in  the  face  of  comparative 
chaos  within  the  physical  environment.  Ambiguity  of  [stimulus] 
cues  and  means  [i.e.,  organismic  responses]  relative  to  the  vitally 
relevant  objects  and  results  must  find  its  counterpart  in  an  ambiguity 
and  flexibility  of  the  .  .  .  mediating  processes  in  the  organism."  ^  Or, 
to  use  Whitehead's  words,  ",  .  .  every  scheme  for  the  analysis  of  na- 
ture has  to  face  these  two  facts  change  and  endurance^  The  mo- 
mentum of  the  logic  we  are  using  in  this  book  has  led  us  to  define  two 
basic  maladjustive  factors  in  terms  of  these  dichotomous  sources  of 
anxiety:  rigidity,  which  brings  a  narrow  adjustment  to  one  aspect  of 
the  environment,  and  unstable  oscillation  which  is  an  intense  attempt 
to  adjust  to  all  aspects  of  the  presented  environment.  These  concepts 
involve  the  multilevel  organization  and  diagnosis  of  personality  and 
must  await  publication  in  a  subsequent  volume. 

Between  the  two  maladjustive  extremes  of  personality,  rigid  con- 
tinuity and  oscillating  noncontinuity,  occur  the  greatest  majority  of 
human  adjustments.  Most  individuals,  as  we  have  seen,  tend  to  select 
a  limited  set  of  preferred  reflexes  which  operate  spontaneously,  but 
not  with  inflexible  repetition.  The  average  individual  is  still  able  to 
call  out  automatically  any  and  all  reflexes  along  the  continuum  to  meet 
the  exigencies  of  the  environment.  In  general  orientation  and  in  the 
crucial  decisions  of  his  life,  he  is  likely,  however,  to  have  employed 
the  narrowed  responses.  And  he  has  very  likely  succeeded  in  training 
the  significant  "others"  in  his  life  to  react  in  reciprocity  to  his  inter- 
personal style.  The  average  person  has  thus  created  himself  and  his 
world  along  the  lines  of  a  purposive  but  limited  set  of  interpersonal 
relationships.  He  has  worked  out,  usually  by  means  of  involuntary 
reflexes,  a  balance  which  is  best  calculated  to  meet  the  double  threats 
of  rigidity  and  chaotic  flexibility.  His  Level  I  automatic  communica- 
tions have  provided  him  with  smoothly  operating  techniques  (^e- 

^  The  italics  and  parenthetic  notes  are  the  author's. 


122  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

termining  the  nature  of  his  self  and  world.  Like  his  more  neurotic 
brethren,  he  too  gets  from  life  the  interpersonal  returns  for  which  he 
has  bargained — just  that  and  no  more. 

The  Principle  of  Reciprocal  Interpersonal  Relations 

The  principle  of  self-determination  as  it  operates  at  Level  I  has 
several  implications.  The  notion  that  we  must  take  the  credit  or  blame 
for  our  own  life  situations  has  had  an  obvious  effect  on  clinical  prac- 
tice. It  assigns  to  the  individual  patient  the  responsibility  for  develop- 
ing and  managing  his  own  personality.  This  is  a  terrible  power  that 
we  assign  to  him,  one  which  he  is  often  not  willing  to  believe  or  accept. 
The  key  factors  in  personality  seem  to  be  the  purposive  messages  we 
express  to  others  in  our  Level  I  communications.  For  many  patients 
these  are  signals  of  weakness  and  blame:  "Others  must  help  me"  and 
"Others  are  my  undoing"  are  familiar  and  poignant  themes  expressed 
by  many  psychiatric  patients.  The  notion  of  self-determination  re- 
moves the  protective  devices  of  projection  and  externalization — giving 
in  return  a  priceless,  but  often  unwelcome  gift  of  personal  power. 

In  developing  these  themes  a  rather  curious  imbalance  may  have 
been  noted.  For  purposes  of  exposition  we  have  concentrated  on  the 
viewpoint  of  the  subject.  At  times  it  may  have  implied  a  paradoxical 
situation  in  which  everyone  goes  around  training  others  to  respond  to 
him  in  specified  ways.  This  is,  of  course,  rather  puzzling.  If  everyone 
is  actively  creating  his  own  interpersonal  world,  this  leaves  no  one 
left  to  be  passively  trained  by  others. 

This  dilemma  is  caused  by  the  concentration  on  one  side  of  the 
interpersonal  exchange — the  subject.  Actually,  we  know  that  we  can 
never  understand  interpersonal  relationships  unless  we  study  both  sides 
of  the  interaction.  When  we  pause  to  isolate  and  study  one  side — the 
self  or  subject  side — of  interpersonal  behavior,  we  do  so  at  the  risk  of 
distortion.  As  we  consider,  in  turn,  the  various  levels  and  areas,  we 
encounter  the  danger  of  segmental  overemphasis — one  of  the  plague? 
of  psychological  theory.  The  principle  of  self-determination  is  a 
probability  statement  which  has  reference  to  the  global  organization  of 
personality  in  general  and  Level  I  in  particular.  The  over-all  system 
of  the  total  personality  is  for  all  predictive  purposes  the  unit  upon 
which  we  focus.  It  has  special  importance  in  shaping  a  strategy  and 
tactic  of  psychotherapy.  It  should  be  kept  clear  that  in  the  preceding 
section  we  have,  for  expository  purposes,  stressed  the  "self"  response 
and  understressed  the  "other,"  or  environmental  factors.  In  actuality 
both  partners  in  any  relationship  share  the  responsibility  for  its  de- 
velopment— a  mutual  determining  operation  is  occurring.  The  mother 
does  not  create  the  child's  personality.  The  child  does  not  create  the 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  123 

maternal  reaction.  They  both  are  engaged  in  a  most  intricate  recipro- 
cal process  to  which  both  bring  determinative  motivations. 

Many  Interpersonal  Exchanges  Reinforce  the  Original  Re- 
flex. The  time  has  now  come  to  consider  both  sides  of  the  interper- 
sonal situation — the  two-person  commerce  of  communication.  The 
first  point  worth  comment  is  the  reinforcing  quality  of  social  interac- 
tion. Our  actions  toward  other  people  generally  have  the  effect  of 
pulling  a  reciprocal  response  from  them.  This  in  turn  tends  to 
strengthen  our  original  action.  If  you  walk  up  and  aggressively  shove 
a  stranger,  the  chances  are  good  that  he  will  shove  you  back.  Of 
course,  this  rule  does  not  work  uniformly.  One  out  of  a  hundred 
might  be  that  Christian  soul  who  would  tenderly  embrace  you.  A 
few  might  slink  away  from  you.  A  few  might  docilely  attempt  to 
placate  you.  The  largest  percentage  would  mirror  your  aggression — 
and  probably  shove  back.  Your  counterresponse  then  becomes  the 
issue.  You  might  apologize,  you  might  retreat,  but  assuming  you  are 
an  "aggressive  shover"  to  begin  with,  the  statistically  probable  re- 
sponse is  to  shove  back,  perhaps  harder. 

You  have  provoked  a  response  which  has  reinforced  your  original 
action.  This  reinforcing  process  has  been  dignified  with  the  title  of 
the  principle  of  reciprocal  interpersonal  relations.  This  is  a  general 
probability  principle.  It  holds  that:  Interpersonal  reflexes  tend  {with 
a  probability  significantly  greater  than  chance)  to  initiate  or  invite 
reciprocal  interpersonal  responses  from  the  ^^other^^  person  in  the  inter- 
action that  lead  to  a  repetition  of  the  original  reflex. 

Before  considering  the  ramifications  and  quaUfications  of  this  prin- 
ciple we  shall  glance  at  a  few  examples  of  its  operation. 

How  Group  Therapy  Patients  Provoke  Each  Other  to  In- 
creasing Repetition.  Group  psychotherapy  provides  a  splendid  op- 
portunity to  observe  the  development  of  interpersonal  patterns.  The 
members  come  together  as  strangers.  The  initial  sessions  are  anxiety- 
provoking.  The  novelty  and  tension  combine  to  produce  interpersonal 
reflex  behavior  which  is  quite  clear-cut.  Unless  a  definite  selection 
principle  is  employed,  the  members  of  a  group  tend  to  scatter  around 
the  interpersonal  circle.  That  is,  one  or  two  will  demonstrate  helpless, 
dependent  reflexes.  One  or  two  will  be  sullen  and  silent.  One  or  two 
will  be  superior,  mildly  antagonistic.  One  or  two  will  briskly  begin 
to  take  leadership  roles,  try  to  "get  the  ball  rolling,"  help  the  other 
patients,  etc.  The  pattern  of  reciprocal  reflexes  that  develops  is  rich 
and  complex. 

For  demonstration  purposes  it  might  be  best  to  review  a  less  hetero- 
geneous group.  We  think  here  of  the  group  comprising  three  phobic, 


124  "^"^  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

dependent  women,  two  schizoid  men,  and  a  psychosomatic  man.  In 
the  first  session  the  three  fearful  women  nervously  described  their 
symptoms  and  then  fell  into  a  protective  silence.  The  schizoid  men 
muttered  their  introductions  and  sank  into  an  isolated  retreat.  The 
floor  was  left  to  the  therapist  and  the  psychosomatic  man. 

The  latter  was  a  friendly,  energetic,  talkative  person  who  rattled 
on  for  about  ten  minutes  about  his  symptoms  and  his  life  situation. 
When  he  finished  his  competent  and  congenial  narrative,  the  group 
fell  into  a  prolonged  silence.  After  two  or  three  intensely  long  minutes 
of  soundless  hush  the  psychosomatic  patient  entered  again  with  a  ques- 
tion to  the  therapist.  The  patient  then  expanded  on  this  topic  for  about 
five  minutes.  The  tomblike  silence  resumed — interrupted  only  by  the 
shifting  of  chairs  and  the  rustle  of  smoking  activity. 

The  therapist  then  intervened  to  comment  on  the  silence  and  asked 
each  patient  in  turn  what  his  associations  were  to  the  topic  introduced 
by  the  psychosomatic  patient.  The  therapist  concluded  the  session 
by  reviewing  the  silence  and  explaining  that  he  had  intervened  at  the 
end  to  help  the  patients  learn  how  to  communicate  in  the  group. 

During  the  second  meeting  the  same  pattern  repeated.  Long  silences 
developed.  The  tension  clearly  mounted  during  these  lulls.  The 
phobic  ladies  squirmed,  looked  uncomfortable,  but  kept  silent.  The 
schizoid  men  frowned,  edged  their  chairs  further  toward  the  corners, 
and  kept  silent.  The  pressure  on  the  friendly,  talkative  member  would 
build  up  until  he  would  finally  begin  to  speak.  He  tried  to  get  the 
others  to  talk.  He  asked  them  questions.  He  described  at  greater  and 
greater  length  events  from  his  own  life  (most  of  them  concerning 
superficial  events — hobbies,  work  experiences,  etc.).  By  the  fourth 
session  the  tension  had  mounted  to  an  intense  peak.  The  silences  grew 
longer  and  more  painful.  The  psychosomatic  patient  found  himself 
involved  in  a  series  of  monologues.  It  seemed  that  the  patients  were 
all  getting  disgusted  with  themselves  and  with  each  other,  the  one  for 
talking  too  much  and  the  others  for  not  talking  enough.  By  this  point 
the  talkative  patient,  in  fact,  found  it  hard  to  refrain  from  talking. 
When  one  of  the  others  would  venture  a  comment  he  would  inter- 
rupt, ask  questions,  and  relate  his  own  associations.  The  executive 
outgoing  patient  had  trained  the  others  to  be  listeners.  He  later  con- 
fessed that  he  rather  fancied  himself  as  a  subleader  in  the  group,  and 
half-boastingly,  half-sheepishly  described  his  reflex  skills  in  extro- 
verted glibness. 

The  other  group  patients  had  successfully  trained  the  extrovert  to 
dominate  them.  They  had  forced  him  into  a  responsible,  competent 
role  and  had  thus  emphasized  and  reinforced  their  own  withdrawing 
tendencies.  Their  original  reflex  patterns  had  contributed  to  a  tense 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  125 

situation.  The  more  the  tension  developed,  the  more  they  increased 
their  reflex  techniques  for  handling  anxiety.  The  psychosomatic  pa- 
tient was  almost  frantically  active  and  the  others  silent.  They  were  all 
making  a  failure  out  of  the  group  along  the  same  lines  of  their  life 
failures.  When  this  reciprocal  process  had  reached  its  optimal  point, 
the  therapist  intervened  to  help  the  members  to  understand  how  they 
reacted  to  the  tension  and  how  they  increased  it  by  their  reactions. 

We  expect  each  group  member  to  contribute  to  the  failure  of  com- 
munication that  tends  to  develop  in  our  therapy  groups.  This  initial 
breakdown  caused  by  the  reciprocal  principle  is  allowed  to  develop  in 
the  early  stages  in  the  group  since  it  allows  each  patient  to  repeat  his 
interpersonal  imbalances  in  the  therapy  situation.  The  very  real  ten- 
sion of  the  group  situation  provides  valuable  information  about  how 
each  person  handles  anxiety.  It  is  somewhat  analogous  to  the  trans- 
ference neurosis  of  individual  therapy.  It  provides  material  for  many 
months  of  subsequent  analysis. 

This  example  centered  around  the  reciprocal  patterns  of  one  mem- 
ber versus  the  group.  More  discrete  interactions  between  pairs  of  in- 
dividual patients  inevitably  develop  and  pro\ide  more  complex  and 
specific  examples  of  the  reciprocal  process.  Earlier  in  this  chapter  we 
have  described  a  distrustful  patient  who  was  convinced  that  others 
were  unsympathetic  and  mean  to  him.  He  provides  another  example 
of  the  reciprocal  principle.  We  saw  how  this  man  easily  and  auto- 
matically provoked  rejection  and  dislike  by  means  of  his  growling 
suspiciousness.  The  hostile  reception  he  received  from  the  others  led, 
of  course,  to  an  increase  in  his  bitter  distrust.  He  invited  responses 
which  led  to  a  repetition  of  his  original  reflex  pattern. 

Reciprocal  Relations  Are  Probable,  Not  Inevitable.  The  re- 
inforcing process  we  have  been  describing  is  not  an  all-inclusive  prin- 
ciple. It  is  a  probability  function.  It  does  not  necessarily  hold  for  the 
individual  interaction.  Aggression  usually  breeds  counteraggression. 
Smiles  usually  win  smiles.  Tears  usually  provoke  sympathy.  In  spe- 
cific cases,  however,  these  general  rules  break  down.  Aggression  can 
win  tolerant  smiles.  Tears  can  provoke  curses.  But,  when  we  study 
the  thousands  of  interactions  that  make  up  each  day  of  social  existence, 
the  principle  becomes  increasingly  useful.  Many  kinds  of  variation 
and  inconsistency  operate  to  lower  perfect  predictability  of  inter- 
personal behavior.  The  meaning  of  the  cultural  context,  the  personal- 
ity of  the  "other  one,"  and  oscillation  tendencies  in  the  individual  are 
always  complicating  factors.  Like  any  other  principle  which  involves 
human  emotions,  the  principle  of  reciprocal  relations  operates  in 
probabilistic  terms. 


126  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

Effect  of  the  Other  Person's  Personality.  Reciprocal  rela- 
tions are  more  likely- to  develop  with  certain  personalities.  The  prin- 
ciple holds  most  uniformly  with  pairs  of  symbiotically  "sick"  people. 
A  phobic,  dependent  wife  and  a  nurturant,  strong  husband  would  be 
such  a  pair.  The  more  the  husband  takes  care  of  her,  the  more  the  de- 
pendence repeats.  The  more  the  wife  clings,  the  more  pressure  on  the 
husband  to  be  gentle  and  protective.  Even  in  a  symbiotic  marriage  of 
this  sort,  the  reciprocity  would  tend  to  break  down  if  other  motives 
enter  the  behavior  of  either.  If  hostile  reproach  lies  behind  the  wife's 
weakness,  or  impatient  superiority  behind  the  husband's  strength,  then 
new  chains  of  interaction  may  develop. 

Another  aspect  of  this  principle:  The  sicker  you  are  the  more 
power  you  have  to  determine  the  relationships  you  have  with  others. 
A  maladjusted  person  with  a  crippled  set  of  reflexes  tends  to  over- 
develop a  narrow  range  of  one  or  two  interpersonal  responses.  These 
are  expressed  intensely  and  often,  whether  appropriate  to  the  situa- 
tion or  not.  Now  a  normal  person  has  a  fairly  flexible  range  of  re- 
flexes. He  can  use  any  interpersonal  response  if  the  situation  calls  it 
out.  He  is  less  committed  to  and,  for  that  matter,  less  skillful  in  the 
use  of  any  particular  reflex.  When  the  two  interact,  it  is  the  "sick" 
person  who  determines  the  relationship. 

Suppose  that  the  suspicious  young  man  just  cited  meets  up  with  a 
fairly  well-rounded  person.  The  latter  may  greet  him  cheerfully.  The 
other  may  frown,  or  shoot  a  sharp  glance,  then  cast  his  eyes  to  the 
ground.  The  normal  person  may  invite  the  other  to  the  movies — to 
which  he  replies  with  a  sullen  remark.  No  matter  how  flexible  or  well- 
meaning  the  one  may  be,  the  other  will  eventually  force  him  to  take  a 
negative  critical  position. 

The  more  extreme  and  rigid  the  person,  the  greater  his  interper- 
sonal "pull" — the  stronger  his  ability  to  shape  the  relationships  with 
others.  The  withdrawn  catatonic,  the  irretrievable  criminal,  the 
compulsively  flirtatious  charmer  can  inevitably  provoke  the  expected 
response  from  a  more  well-balanced  "other." 

The  flexible  person  can  pull  a  greater  variety  of  responses  from 
others — depending  on  his  conscious  or  unconscious  motives  at  the 
moment.  He  can  get  others  to  hke  him,  take  care  of  him,  obey  him, 
lead  him,  envy  him,  etc.  The  "sick"  person  has  a  very  narrow  range  of 
interpersonal  tactics,  but  these  are  generally  quite  powerful  in  their 
effect.  I  have  seen  compulsive,  responsible  group  members  after  sev- 
eral months  of  treatment  desperately  trying  to  get  the  other  group 
members  to  understand  and  commiserate  with  their  inner  feelings  of 
weakness  and  despair.   They  had  trained  them  well  to  look  up  and 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


127 


respect  them.  Their  own  managerial  reflexes  kept  firing  even  at  the 
moment  they  were  verbally  appealing  for  help  and  sympathy. 

Variation  Within  the  Individual  Affects  Reciprocal  Rela- 
tionships. Another  qualification  of  the  principle  of  reciprocal  rela- 
tions must  be  included.  In  describing  human  behavior  the  impression 
is  often  given  that  a  consistent  line  of  adjustment  is  exhibited.  In  most 
of  the  illustrations  used  in  this  chapter,  the  subject's  role  is  made  to  ap- 
pear fixed.  Actually,  we  know  that  inconsistency  and  changeability 
are  the  rule  and  not  the  exception  in  human  emotions.  The  factors  of 
change  and  stability  will  come  under  detailed  survey  in  Chapter  13. 
They  are,  indeed,  studied  as  a  separate  dimension  of  personality — the 
variability  dimension.  Included  under  this  topic  are  all  the  measurable 
variations  which  affect  human  behavior — changes  in  cultural  context, 
changes  over  time,  changes  due  to  conflict  and  variety  among  the 
levels  of  personality. 

At  this  point  it  is  sufficient  to  point  out  that  no  interpersonal  role 
is  absolutely  pure  or  rigid.  The  most  withdrawn  catatonic  sends  out 
occasional  tendrils  of  affect.  The  most  hardened  criminal  occasionally 
has  a  moment  of  congeniality.  The  most  autocratic  five-star  general 
occasionally  admits  he  is  wrong.  Most  people  show  considerable  con- 
flict or  inconsistency  in  their  actions  from  time  to  time.  No  matter 
how  thick  and  effective  the  reflex  defenses,  underlying  inconsistencies 
eventually  manifest  themselves. 

When  this  happens  the  principle  of  reciprocal  relations  tends  to 
break  down.  The  probable  accuracy  of  the  predictions  drops.  A 
flirtatious  woman  provokes  seductive  responses  from  a  man.  His  ap- 
proaches set  off  stronger  flirtatious  actions.  The  man  becomes  more 
seductive.  At  some  point  in  this  process  underlying  motives  may  step 
in  to  change  the  pattern.  In  some  cases,  a  flirtatious  fa9ade  may  cover 
deeper  feelings  of  competition  or  contempt  toward  men.  The  woman 
would  then  shift  to  behavior  which  Erickson  describes  as  "bitchy," 
and  rejecting.  The  reciprocal  pattern  of  entice  versus  seduce  would 
shift.  The  man's  reaction  would  then  vary  depending  on  the  nature 
of  his  multilevel  pattern.  He  might  continue  to  seduce,  he  might  be 
hurt,  he  might  become  dependent. 

The  same  process  of  circular  interactions  leading  up  to  an  intense 
breaking  point  often  occurs  between  parent  and  child.  Dependence 
pulls  nurturance  which  provokes  further  dependence — .  In  some 
cases  the  spiraling  increase  in  intensity  leads  to  a  temporary  crash.  At 
some  point  the  parent's  underlying  feelings  of  selfishness  or  self-pro- 
tection lead  to  refusal.    Father  comes  home  one  night  tired  and 


128  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

grumpy.  Outside  events  may  have  set  off  underlying  feelings  of  dep- 
rivation, or  self-pity,  or  sadism.  He  may  snarl  at  the  child.  The  child 
then  whines.  The  whining  might  increase  the  father's  irritation.  A 
new  series  of  reciprocal  events  may  thus  be  initiated. 

Alternation  of  behavior  is,  of  course,  not  an  unhealthy  manifesta- 
tion. Moods  shift;  we  carry  over  the  feelings  from  one  situation  into 
another.  Events  of  the  day  set  off  underlying  effects  which  may  be 
quite  different  from  the  current  reality  situation.  It  is  safe  to  suggest 
that  everyone  acts  inappropriately  many  times  each  day.  These  incon- 
sistencies can  hardly  be  considered  abnormal.  The  lines  of  inter- 
personal communication  are  constantly  breaking  down  momentarily, 
but  these  involve  no  permanent  disasters.  A  healthy  father-child  re- 
lationship is  not  paralyzed  because  one  of  the  two  has  a  "bad  day"  or 
carries  over  inappropriate  effects. 

On  the  other  hand,  very  rigidly  formed  relationships  can  be  upset 
badly  by  shifts  in  the  pattern  of  reciprocal  relations.  Some  institu- 
tional relationships  are  very  inflexible  and  demand  perfect  reciprocity. 
The  army  officer  expects  to  provoke  consistent  obedience.  A  rent  in 
this  kind  of  interpersonal  fabric  can  be  seen  as  unforgivable.  Some 
kinds  of  symbiotic  marriages  are  so  rigid  that  deviation  in  reciprocal 
roles  can  cause  intense  anxiety.  When  a  servile,  docile  husband  shows 
a  flash  of  rebellion  against  a  dominating  wife,  the  results  can  be  ex- 
plosive. 

Thus,  we  see  that  many  factors  tend  to  qualify  the  principle  of 
reciprocal  relations.  Among  these  we  have  considered  variations  in 
the  cultural  context,  variations  in  the  personality  of  the  "other  one," 
and  variations  due  to  multilevel  ambivalences  in  the  subject's  personal- 
ity. 

Multilevel  Reciprocity  Patterns.  We  have  very  little  system- 
atic knowledge  about  interpersonal  relations.  We  do  know  that  a 
most  complex,  shifting  matrix  of  forces  operates  in  the  simplest  inter- 
action. Throughout  this  book  we  are  forced  to  limit  the  theory,  the 
illustrations,  and  the  measurements  to  the  simplest  forms  of  interac- 
tion. One  example  of  a  rich  and  vital  phenomenon  which  is  at  present 
beyond  reach  of  our  system  has  to  do  with  multilevel  reciprocal  pat- 
terns. Complex  patterns  of  interaction  exist  at  all  the  levels  of  per- 
sonality. In  some  cases  the  smooth  flowing  exchanges  of  one  level  are 
threatened  and  destroyed  by  clashes  caused  by  underlying  variations. 
That  is,  two  people  may  interact  in  a  most  automatic  and  rewarding 
pattern  at  the  level  of  the  interpersonal  reflex.  The  seductive  man  and 
the  flirtatious  woman  is  one  such  situation.  At  the  private  level  the 
feelings  of  both  partners  may  be  quite  different.  The  man  may  have 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION 


129 

000      S'SSS-      S!='55~ 


a  o      00000 


lEuijoiuadXH      Q 


|BUOnU3AU03J3AO 

-3AnEJ3d003 


OrrsOo       >Ot1-  —  \o       (^iO»viirs       OOOO'*- 


U 


60 


h 


O   O")   r.-,    — .         >0   "r^ 


Q  h  ^     '-.  r-i  '■'    (^1       vo  r-  t-~  ^O       r<-,  r^  r^  ./^       TA  O  r-4  — '  O 

^  -SUIDEi^a-JpS  a::    "         -  -  ^       rv. 


g  I                                 OUSipeS        ttj    „  ,,  o  ^  O  rv,  .^  ^.  ^  00  r^.  O  r.  O  .^.  -.  O 

g  h.                          -3AISS3J33V  Q    "^         "^  "                                                               -'  -' 

tJ       ^  OUSISSpjB^I        Ov.0  000  Or-1-.-f  O^fMOO  r^--^-^0 

2     """  -3Ann3duio3  *           ~'  ~'  "■                    n  —  — 


>^     i-v,  1^,  rn  — I       — .  r^  _■  <> 


o  ^ 

w  4J  ii      2  -7;  1J' 


E 

^  ^  S  S  = 

j3  -  J^  c  (u 

C  -S  Qj  •-  '*^ 


IS  ^ 


1  "O 


lilts  i^ii  i«=si  §1  .^1 

I  .2  «  e  0.    >>^  -i  I,  -5  '^  e  «   '2  '5  -f  2  2 

S    ^^  <=^      >-Sc>:.     "2;^  >^  4j     c  c  o  o  (£ 


,  30  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

deeper  sadistic  motives  toward  women.  The  flirtatious  girl  may  have 
underlying  needs  to  reject  and  humiliate  men.  What  starts  off  as  a 
most  spontaneous  and  fluid  friendship  eventually  ends  in  a  brawl. 
The  underlying  feelings  of  the  participants  do  not  lend  themselves 
to  a  durable  relationship. 

This  aggressive  ending  might  not  occur  if  the  underlying  feelings  of 
the  partners  were  reciprocal.  Suppose  that  the  woman's  underlying 
feelings,  instead  of  being  competitive  and  rejecting,  were  masochistic. 
Their  surface  reflexes  blend  nicely  into  the  pattern  of  seduce  and  en- 
tice. Their  deeper  feelings  would,  in  this  case,  also  blend  nicely  into 
the  reciprocal  pattern  of  sadism-masochism.  The  man's  preconscious 
aggressiveness  would  tailor  nicely  into  the  woman's  private  needs  to 
be  aggressed  upon.  It  is  possible  that  many  symbiotic  marriages  exist 
in  which  multilevel  needs  of  both  mates  fit  together  into  multilevel 
patterns  of  reciprocity.  Our  measurement  methods  are  far  from  being 
able  to  tap  these  intricate  networks  which  seem  to  characterize  even 
the  simplest  relationships. 

Incidence  of  Level  I-M  Behavior  in  Various  Cultural  Sam- 
ples. A  summary  of  the  research  findings  concerning  Level  I  be- 
havior is  presented  after  each  of  the  eight  clinical  chapters  (Chapters 
15-22).  At  this  point,  to  give  an  overview,  it  may  be  helpful  to  list 
the  percentage  of  Level  I-M  types  found  in  several  institutional  or 
symptomatic  samples. 

In  the  preceding  chapter  a  method  was  described  for  summarizing 
interpersonal  behavior  at  any  level  in  terms  of  a  single  point  on  the 
diagnostic  grid.  The  location  of  this  summary  point  determines  the 
interpersonal  diagnosis  (see  Chapter  12).  Thus  if  the  resultant  of  the 
Level  I  scores  for  an  individual  locates  in  the  AP  octant  he  is  diag- 
nosed as  a  Managerial-Autocratic  personality  at  this  level.  Table  2 
presents  the  percentage  of  cases  in  fifteen  samples  falling  in  each 
octant  at  Level  I-M. 

A  detailed  re-examination  of  these  data  will  be  found  in  later  chap- 
ters, but  some  of  the  meaning  of  Level  I-M  behavior  can  be  derived 
by  inspection  of  the  table.  Some  samples  (military  officers,  normals, 
psychosomatic  cases)  emphasize  strong,  hypernormal  facades  (octants 
AP  and  NO).  Those  samples  which  include  people  in  trouble  (prison- 
ers, psychiatric  patients)  manifest  more  alienated  or  passive  behavior 
(octants  DE,  FG,  or  HI). 

References 

1.  Bales,  R.  F.    Interaction  process  analysis.    Cambridge,  Mass.:    Addison-Wesley 
Press,  1950. 

2.  BiON,  W.  R.  Experience  in  groups:  III.  Human  Relations,  1949,  11,  No.  1,  13-22. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PUBLIC  COMMUNICATION  131 

3.  Brunswik,  E.  The  conceptual  framework  of  psychology.  International  encyclo- 
pedia of  unified  science,  Vol.  /,  No.  10,  Chicago:  University  of  Chicago  Press, 
1952.  Copyright  1952  by  The  University  of  Chicago. 

4.  Cassirer,  E.  An  essay  on  man.  New  Haven:  Yale  University  Press,  1944. 

5.  Lawrence,  D.  H.  Women  in  love.  Modern  Library  ed.  New  York:  Random 
House,  Inc.,  1920. 

6.  Leary,  T.,  and  H.  Coffey.  The  prediction  of  interpersonal  behavior  in  group 
psychotherapy.  Psychodrama  and  gr.  psychother.  Monogr.,  1955,  No.  28. 

7.  Mead,  G.  H.  Mind,  self  and  society.  Chicago:  University  of  Chicago  Press,  1934. 
Copyright  1934  by  The  University  of  Chicago. 

8.  Powelson,  D.  H.,  and  R.  Bendix.  Psychiatry  in  prison.  Fsychiat.,  1951,  14,  73-86. 

9.  Sapir,  E.  Speech  as  a  personality  trait.  Amer.  J.  Sociol.,  1927,  32,  892-905,  Uni- 
versity of  Chicago  Press.  Copyright  1927  by  The  University  of  Chicago. 

10.  Sw^EET,  Blanche.  A  study  of  insight:  its  operational  definition  and  its  relation- 
ship to  psychological  health.  Unpublished  doctor's  dissertation,  University  of 
California,  Berkeley,  1953. 

11.  Thelen,  H.  a.  Method  of  sequential  analysis  of  group  process.  Mimeographed 
working  instructions,  1952. 

12.  Whitehead,  A.  N.  Science  and  the  modem  world.  New  York:  Macmillan,  1925. 


8 


The  Level  of  Conscious  Communication: 
The  Interpersonal  Trait 


This  chapter  is  devoted  to  an  examination  of  Level  II,  the  data  of  con- 
scious description.  We  deal  here  with  the  individual's  perceptions  of 
himself  and  his  world  as  he  reports  them. 

Like  the  other  levels  of  personality,  this  one  is  automatically  defined 
by  the  data  which  contribute  to  it.  There  is  only  one  criterion  for  de- 
termining Level  II  data:  conscious  verbal  report  by  the  subject.  We 
are  interested  in  ivhat  the  subject  says,  the  content  of  his  verbal  ex- 
pressions. From  these  we  focus  on  the  interpersonal  themes  which  he 
attributes  to  himself  and  to  "others."  From  these  we  obtain  the 
variables  of  Level  11. 

It  must  be  noted  that  the  consensual  accuracy  or  truth  of  these 
verbal  reports  has  no  bearing  on  the  definition  of  the  level.  If  the  sub- 
ject says  he  is  popular,  the  appropriate  Level  II  code  for  this  inter- 
personal role  is  assigned.  Now  dozens  of  observers  may  agree  that 
he  is  quite  unpopular  with  his  associates.  This  fact  shows  up  on  our 
diagrams  for  Level  I.  But  the  Level  II  rating  concerns  not  what  he 
does,  not  what  he  privately  thinks  or  wishes,  but  what  he  says. 

This  is  called  the  level  of  conscious  description  because  it  reflects 
how  the  subject  chooses  to  present  himself  and  his  view  of  the  world. 
It  will  be  noted  that  we  do  not  call  it  the  level  of  consciousness,  but  of 
conscious  communication.  This  is  an  important  distinction.  The 
phenomenon  of  consciousness  is  one  of  the  most  elusive  issues  in  the 
history  of  Western  thought.  One  of  its  most  confusing  aspects  is,  of 
course,  its  subjective  nature.  The  scientist  can  never  understand  or 
measure  what  another  person  has  in  his  consciousness.  It  is  often  quite 
difficult  for  the  subject  himself  to  know  the  focus  and  limits  or  his 
awareness.  Between  the  subject  and  the  psychologist  there  exists  any 
number  of  potentially  distorting  factors — deliberate  omissions,  expres- 

132 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION  1^3 

sive  inaccuracies,  and  the  like.  And  we  never  know  the  exact  level  of 
awareness  from  which  the  statements  come. 

Since  it  is  impossible  to  obtain  an  objective  evaluation  of  the  sub- 
jective viewpoint  of  another  person,  many  psychologists  have  at- 
tempted to  discard  the  whole  issue  of  consciousness.  But  in  so  doing 
an  essential  dimension  of  human  behavior  is  lost. 

Two  principles  must  be  applied  to  any  scientific  approach  to  the 
conscious  aspects  of  pei-sonality.  The  first  is  the  classic  solution  de- 
veloped (but  not  utilized)  by  the  earliest  behaviorists:  treat  the  sub- 
ject's introspection  not  as  the  essence  of  truth,  but  as  a  behavioral  ex- 
pression to  be  evaluated  in  the  light  of  all  the  other  measurements. 
The  second  principle  is  an  explicit  corollary  that  can  only  develop 
from  a  systematic  multilevel  analysis  of  behavior.  It  holds  that  the 
data  of  conscious  report  have  of  themselves  an  ambiguous  meaning 
until  they  are  systematically  evaluated  in  the  light  of  the  data  from 
the  other  levels  of  behavior. 

At  Level  II  we  deal,  therefore,  with  conscious  reports  and  not  con- 
sciousness. We  define  it  operationally  in  terms  of  all  the  statements 
an  individual  makes  about  himself  or  his  world.  We  employ  it  and 
evaluate  it  in  relation  to  other  levels  of  personality. 

The  Attributive  Nature  of  Personality  Language 

Transcriptions  of  everything  that  a  patient  says  during  an  hour  of 
psychotherapy  provide  one  source  of  raw  data  for  Level  II  measure- 
ments. The  patient's  testimony  in  this  form  can  then  be  studied  from 
the  standpoint  of  the  interpersonal  system.  Everything  that  the  patient 
says  about  himself  becomes  Level  II  "Self."  Everything  he  says  about 
the  people  in  his  interpersonal  world  becomes  Level  II  "Other."  The 
accuracy,  the  deeper  significance,  the  immediate  purpose  behind  these 
responses  is  disregarded.  Their  direct  surface  meaning  is  the  essence 
of  the  Level  II  classification. 

In  studying  the  verbal  content  of  these  descriptions  an  interesting 
fact  develops.  They  are  all  attributive  or  adjectival.  They  are  all 
significations.  They  can  all  be  interpreted  as  assigning  a  quality  to  the 
self  or  the  world.  They  can,  thus,  be  reduced  for  analysis  to  a  descrip- 
tive adjective  or  to  adjectival  phrases.  The  interpersonal  context  of 
everything  that  is  said  about  oneself  or  one's  world  can  be  translated 
into  a  generic  attributive  form:  "I  am  a person,  in  relation- 
ship to 

Take,  for  example,  the  patient's  testimony,  "I  was  really  angry  at 
my  boss  today.  I  took  it  docilely  for  a  while.  Finally  I  insulted  him. 
I've  always  hated  his  guts."  The  subject  and  object  of  this  inter- 
personal relationship  are  clearly  self-boss.   The  four  sentences  vary 


1 34  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

in  their  mode  of  expressing  the  interpersonal  theme — adjective,  verb, 
participle — but  they  can  all  be  translated  into  the  attributive  formula: 

angry       -| 

I  feel  like  an:     insulrine-   I   P^^^^"  ^^  relationship  to  my  boss, 
hating       J 

At  the  level  of  conscious  description  we  deal  with  the  subject's 
language  about  himself  and  others.  The  interpersonal  attributes  are 
the  specific  rated  units.  The  interpersonal  themes  expressed  in  these 
significations  are  coded  according  to  the  matrix  of  sixteen  variables 
and  provide  a  systematic  summary  of  the  subject's  view  of  himself  and 
his  world.  The  operational  procedure  for  defining  a  level  in  terms  of 
the  source  of  the  language  is  not  unique  to  this  level.  It  is  now  pos- 
sible to  look  back  at  the  Level  I  reflex  communications  and  see  that 
they  are  also  defined  by  the  source  of  the  language — the  attributive  sig- 
nifications of  the  observers  who  rate  the  subject's  behavior.  At  Level 
I  we  do  not  deal  with  the  reflex  conversation  of  gestures  itself  but 
measure  its  effect  on  others.  We  ask  the  individuals  who  observe  or 
interact  with  the  subject  to  make  attributive  statements  about  him 
which  reflect  his  social  stimulus  value.  The  language  by  which  the 
subject  is  described,  by  others  or  by  himself,  comprises  the  data  for 
Levels  I  and  II  respectively. 

The  data  from  Level  I  can  therefore  be  translated  into  the  same 

type  of  atributive  formula.  ''He  is  a  person  in  relation 

to This  systematic^  approach  to  the  linguistics  of  person- 
ality provides  a  direct  method  for  comparing  the  levels  of  personality. 
The  relationships  between  levels — discrepancies,  concordances — de- 
fine another  dimension  of  personality,  the  variability  dimension.  The 
direct  measurement  of  these  mechanisms  which  thus  relate  the  levels 
of  personality  is  made  possible  by  the  rigorous  analysis  of  the  lan- 
guage of  personality. 

The  Measurement  of  Interpersonal  Attributes 

The  unit  with  which  we  measure  the  language  of  conscious  descrip- 
tion is  called  the  interpersonal  attribute  or  the  interpersonal  trait. 
These  terms  have  been  selected  because  they  reflect  the  adjectival  or 
attributive  nature  of  the  Level  II  data.  We  classify  Level  I  behavior 
in  terms  of  interpersonal  reflexes,  gestures,  or  mechanisms.  We  clas- 
sify Level  II  behavior  in  terms  of  the  interpersonal  attribute  or  trait. 

The  interpersonal  trait  of  Level  II  is  formally  defined  as  the  inter- 
personal motive  attributed  by  the  subject  to  himself  or  another  in  his 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


135 


conscious  reports.  Every  discernible  or  ratable  interpersonal  theme 
in  the  content  of  the  individual's  verbalizations  defines  a  unit  of  Level 
II  behavior. 

The  themes  or  categories  employed  are  derived  from  the  circular 
continuum  of  interpersonal  variables.  It  w^ill  be  recalled  that  in  meas- 
uring the  subject's  reflex  behavior  an  inexhaustible  list  of  sample  verbs 
was  held  to  apply  to  each  of  the  sixteen  generic  interpersonal  pur- 
poses. The  same  procedure  is  followed  for  measuring  attributive  be- 
havior of  Level  II.  The  interpersonal  traits  were  developed  by  simply 


.M««^.^. 


MANAGES  OTHERS 

DOMINATING 

BOSSY 


-^<i^ 


/c 


""'^^^^f^^^^ 


^\A  p 


:C 

1 — ~~~--~.^ 

'^i!    ^-C  ' 

(0 
g 

1 

ten 

\io 


% 


%.W9. 


'% 


4i^ 


r*r 

'\i\ 

OF 

E/LI 
/FRI 
E  T 

?  ^ 

/            FONC 

/     EVERYON 

EVERYBODY 

ALLTH 

LO- 

1  > 

J"^e■'s^■     /CAN  BE 
'^'^  /  OBEDIENT 


'^-^V^^'"^^ 

$i^/..., 


^fslSe7^f-  USUALLY 

'^'VCf'^OV./      EASILY  LED 
MODEST 


y^, 


""6 


^(i^"^ 


PASSIVE  AND 

UNAGGRESSIVE 

MEEK/OBEYS   TOO 

WILLINGLY 


0\ 


-Masochistic 


Figure  6.    Interpersonal  Check  List  Illustrating  the  Classification  of  Interpersonal 
Behaviors  into  Sixteen  Variable  Categories. 


,36  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

replacing  the  verbs  of  Level  I  with  the  coordinate  or  appropriate  ad- 
jective. The  adjectives  which  go  with  the  reflex  (i.e.,  verb)  to  com- 
plain would  obviously  include  complaining,  resentful,  bitter,  etc.  The 
adjectives  which  parallel  the  reflex  to  love  would  be  loving,  affection- 
ate, etc.  This  translation  of  verbs  into  adjective  equivalents  is  not  al- 
ways so  Hnguistically  simple.  There  are  many  interpersonal  reflexes 
for  which  equivalent  adjectives  do  not  exist.  Extensive  methodological 
procedures  have  been  carried  out  in  order  to  deal  with  these  technical 
difficulties.  As  a  result  of  these  exploratory  studies,  the  interpersonal 
meaning  of  most  words  in  the  English  language  which  have  a  social 
connotation  has  been  determined  in  terms  of  the  sixteen-point  con- 
tinuum. 

Several  adjectives  characteristic  of  each  generic  interpersonal  trait 
(i.e.,  each  point  on  the  circular  continuum)  are  included  in  Figure  6. 
These  adjectives  are  suggestive  and  illustrative.  They  are  by  no  means 
exhaustive  of  the  entire  range  of  traits  which  fit  each  point  of  the 
circle.  In  analyzing  the  traits  employed  by  an  individual,  we  rate  not 
only  the  kind  but  the  intensity  of  each  attribute.  Extreme,  inap- 
propriate, and  maladjustive  interpersonal  behaviors  are  thus  distin- 
guished from  the  moderate  and  appropriate.  The  general  nature  of 
these  adjustive  and  maladjustive  traits  is  suggested  by  the  words  listed 
in  the  inner  and  outer  rings  respectively  in  Figure  6. 

Four  Methods  for  Measuring  Level  U  Behavior 

In  Chapter  6  it  was  pointed  out  that  several  methods  exist  for  ob- 
taining the  data  for  any  level.  Whenever  Level  II  data  are  being  dis- 
cussed it  is  necessary  to  indicate  the  specific  source — that  is  the  opera- 
tions through  which  the  data  were  derived. 

When  trained  personnel  rate  the  verbal  content  of  diagnostic  inter- 
views, i.e.,  the  patient's  descriptions  of  himself  and  others,  the  result- 
ing data  are  assigned  to  Level  Il-Di.  These  ratings  are  made  from  on- 
the-spot  observations,  notes,  recordings,  or  transcriptions. 

When  trained  raters  judge  the  verbal  content  of  therapy  interviews 
(group  or  individual)  the  descriptions  of  self  and  others  are  coded 
Level  II-Ti. 

Scores  from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  on  which  the 
patient  rates  his  view  of  self  and  others  are  coded  Level  II-C. 

Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  conscious  descriptions  of  self 
and  others  taken  from  autobiographical  essays  written  by  subjects  are 
coded  Level  II- A. 

There  are,  then,  several  methods  for  obtaining  Level  II  data.  The 
essence  of  them  all  is  that  we  get  the  subject  to  describe  himself  and 
others.   These  reported  perceptions  are  then  scored  in  terms  of  the 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION  137 

circular  continuum.  If  the  data  are  obtained  through  fixed,  prepared 
test  stimuh- questionnaires,  check  Hsts,  and  the  hke,  predetermined 
ratings  assigned  to  each  test  item  make  the  scoring  automatic.  If  the 
data  come  from  free  responses — conversations,  interviews,  autobiog- 
raphies— then  two  or  more  trained  technicians  independently  rate 
each  interpersonal  reference. 

The  rating  of  interpersonal  attributes  at  the  level  of  conscious 
description  is  illustrated  in  the  following  examples: 

The  scoring  of  interpersonal  traits  at  the  conscious  level.  To  illustrate  the 
rating  of  interpersonal  traits  at  the  conscious  level,  there  follow  examples  of 
ratings  of  an  adjective  check  list,  the  content  of  therapy  sessions,  and  an  auto- 
biography. 

a.  The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Traits  as  Applied  to  an  Adjective  Check 
List,  Level  II-C: 

Adjective  Trait  Intensity 

stubborn 

proud 

bossy 

good-leader 

reserved 

withdrawn 

b.  The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Traits  as  Applied  to  the  Content  of  Dis- 
cussion of  a  Group  Psychotherapy  Session,  Level  II-Ti: 

Other  Self 

And  since  I've  been  married  I've  been  able  to  make  a  sub- 
stitute, a  transference  of  these  feelings  from  my  mother  to 
my  wife.    I  guess  I  depend  on  my  wife  a  lot,  more  than  I       K-3 

C-3,  A-2  should.  She  is  a  strong  person.  She  admires  strength.  I 
think  it  makes  her  retract,  withdraw  from  me  when  I  am 
dependent  on  her.    This  makes  me  feel  worse.    Then  she       K-3 

C-3,  A-2       tries  to  drive  me.   She's  an  ambitious  person.   It  makes  me 

feel  very  helpless.  This  is  a  real  vicious  circle  and  it  has  me  1-3 
worried. 

c.  The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Traits  as  Applied  to  an  Autobiography, 
Level  II- A: 

Other  Self 

I  guess  I  was  a  very  co-operative  child,  but  this  is  just  from  L-3 
what  I've  heard.  I've  always  been  timid  all  my  life.  This  is  7-3 
especially  true  with  girls.   I  believe  my  parents  realized  this 

A-2  and  often,  especially  my  mother,  tried  to  push  me  a  little 

which  I  resented  and  probably  went  out  of  my  way  to  do  B-3 
the  opposite.  I  have  always  resented  and  still  do,  being  told        F-3 

A-'i  what  to  do  or  obviously  being  led. 


B 

B 

A 

A 

H 

H 

ijg  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

The  simplest  and  most  standardized  method  for  obtaining  an  esti- 
mate of  Level  II  perceptions  is  to  employ  the  Interpersonal  Adjective 
Check  List.  This  test  has  been  specifically  designed  by  Robert  Suczek, 
Rolfe  La  Forge,  and  others  to  fit  the  matrix  of  the  sixteen  interpersonal 
variables.  In  its  present  form  (Form  IV)  it  consists  of  128  adjectives, 
8  for  each  point  on  the  circle  (see  Appendix  2).  The  check  list  is 
calibrated  in  four  degrees  of  intensity  and  the  array  of  adjectives  is 
balanced  according  to  the  expected  frequency  of  usage.  The  patient 
simply  checks  all  of  the  items  which  he  believes  describe  his  behavior. 
Since  each  term  is  already  prescored,  his  responses  automatically 
produce  his  Level  II-C  self-pattern.^  The  patient  can  be  asked  to  use 
the  same  check  list  to  rate  the  significant  "others"  in  his  life.  This 
gives  a  standardized  picture  of  his  own  description  of  self  and  world. 

The  Patient  Diagnoses  Himself 

Let  us  consider  some  illustrations  of  Level  II  measures  from  a  clinic 
patient  with  a  history  of  chronic  maladjustment.  The  data  were  col- 
lected from  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List  Form  IV  on  which 
the  subject  (at  the  time  of  entrance  to  the  psychiatric  clinic)  succes- 
sively rated  himself  and  his  family  members. 


Figure  7.  Level  II-C  Self-Description  of  Illustrative  Subject 
Based  on  Interpersonal  Check  List.  Key:  Radius  of  circle  equals 
16  check  list  words. 


Figure  7  presents  a  diagrammatic  summary  of  his  self-descriptions. 
It  is  clear  that  the  patient  sees  himself  as  an  "unbalanced"  person.  He 
has  consistently  checked  himself  as  being  distrustful,  passively  hostile, 
and  isolated.    The  marked  imbalance  indicates  that  he  claims  these 

*  A  table  of  norms  for  converting  Level  II-C  dominance  and  hostility  indices  into 
standard  scores  will  be  found  in  Appendix  5. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


139 


traits  to  the  extreme  degree,  thereby  diagnosing  himself  as  disturbed 
in  his  interpersonal  behavior.  Turning  our  attention  to  the  inter- 
personal themes  that  he  does  not  attribute  to  himself  (the  blank  area 
of  the  circle)  we  see  that  he  clearly  denies  all  the  affiliative  (X,  L,  M, 
N,  O)  and  strong  assertive  (P,  A,B,C)  feelings. 

We  have  here  the  patient's  self-diagnosis — a  most  important  shce 
of  the  entire  personality  pattern.  The  Level  II-C  self-profile  has 
considerable  clinical  significance.  Several  probabiUty  laws  hold  for 
this  single  measure. 

A  Patient  Diagnoses  His  Family  Members 

The  patient's  descriptions  of  his  family  members  provide  another 
set  of  valuable  data.  Considering  his  view  of  his  father  (Figure  8),  two 
statements  are  immediately  pertinent:  (1)  he  diagnoses  his  father  as  a 
distrustful  (G),  passively  hostile  (F),  and  isolated  (H)  person;  (2)  he 
sees  his  father  as  being  very  much  like  himself.  It  is  important  to  note 


Figure  8.  Level  II-C  Conscious  Description  of  Father  by 
Illustrative  Subject  Based  on  Interpersonal  Check  List.  Key: 
Radius  of  circle  equals  16  check  list  w^ords. 

the  difference  between  these  two  statements.  To  borrow  the  vocab- 
ulary of  the  logician,  the  first  is  a  class  statement  about  a  single  area  of 
personality,  his  view  of  his  father.  The  second  is  a  relationship  state- 
ment comparing  two  discrete  areas  of  personality — Level  II-C  "Self" 
versus  Level  II-C  "Father." 

Turning  to  this  patient's  view  of  his  mother  (Figure  9)  we  observe 
a  contrast.  The  mother  is  seen  as  unyielding  (B),  rejecting  (C),  and 
punitive  (D)  to  an  extreme  degree.  He  does  not  attribute  any  affec- 
tionate or  passive  qualities  to  her.  Comparing  the  view  of  mother  with 


I40  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIA4ENSION 

his  own  self-perception  (shifting  thereby  from  a  class  to  a  relationship 
context  of  discourse),  a  marked  discrepancy  becomes  apparent.  He 
sees  himself  as  being  like  his  father  but  unlike  his  mother. 


Figure  9.  Conscious  Description  of  Mother  by  Illustrative  Sub- 
ject Based  on  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List.  Key:  Radius  of 
circle  equals  16  check  list  words. 

Level  II  Provides  a  Measure  of  Conscious  Identification 

By  inspection  or  by  quantitative  comparison  we  can  determine  the 
similarity-difference  factors  relating  the  self-profile  to  the  Mother  and 
Father  circles  at  Level  IL  These  relationships  comprise  the  network  of 
measurable  phenomena  called  variability  indices,  which  serve  the  func- 
tion of  relating  the  areas  and  levels  of  personahty.  We  have  suggested 
here  that  the  relationships  between  Level  II  Self  and  Level  II  Other 
can  be  called  identification  or  disidentification.  To  illustrate  some  of 
these  relationship  mechanisms  as  they  operate  at  this  level  of  personal- 
ity we  shall  construct  a  diagrammatic  summary  of  this  patient's  Level 
II  perceptions  (see  Figure  10).  The  lines  linking  the  summary  points 
provide  a  linear  index  of  the  arithmetic  discrepancies  between  the  areas 
of  personality  involved.  The  longer  the  line,  the  greater  the  difference 
in  interpersonal  themes  attributed  to  the  persons  in  question.  It  is 
then  possible  to  translate  this  diagram  into  a  verbal  summary  of  the 
Level  II  behavior,  employing  the  useful,  but  semantically  suspect  lan- 
guage of  the  clinic. 

It  might  be  said  that  this  patient  sees  himself  as  exploited  and  re- 
jected in  relationship  with  an  unsympathetic  and  cold  mother.  He  is 
consciously  identified  with  a  weak  and  distrustful  father.  He  is  con- 
sciously disidentified  with  his  mother. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


141 


The  patient's  view  of  his  wife  (see  Figure  10)  adds  another  factor 
to  the  picture.  He  tells  us  that  she  is  a  hard-hearted,  hostile,  and  re- 
jecting person.  He  consciously  equates  his  wife  with  his  mother,  at- 


MANAGERIAL- 


14P; 


Figure  10.  Diagnostic  Summary  Profile  of  Level  II  Self  and  Other  Scores  for 
Illustrative  Patient.  Key:  The  center  of  the  diagnostic  circle  is  determined  by  the 
mean  of  a  clinic  sample  of  800  cases.  The  placement  of  the  three  summary  scores 
is  determined  by  trigonometric  formulas  (see  Chapter  6)  which  yield  horizontal  and 
vertical  indices.  These  are  converted  to  standard  scores.  The  lines  between  the  sum- 
mary points  provide  linear  estimations  of  the  amount  of  conscious  identification  or  dis- 
identification  (see  Chapters  6  and  13). 

tributing  the  same  interpersonal  motives  to  both.  A  wealth  of  clini- 
cal cues  is  summarized  in  the  family  descriptions.  From  them  we  ob- 
tain the  patient's  conscious  diagnosis  of  his  own  oedipal  situation,  his 
marital  relationship,  and  his  relationships  with  three  central  figures  in 
his  life.  Many  probability  laws  hold  for  each  of  these  measures.  His 


^.j,  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

view  of  parents  is  correlated  with  psychiatric  diagnosis,  symptom, 
and  with  the  intensity  and  type  of  underlying  conflict. 

In  one  sense  these  systematic  measurements  are  quite  limited.  A 
clinical  interview  would  give  the  same  data  just  as  easily  and  would 
provide  a  much  more  rich,  specific,  and  sensitive  registry  of  these  facts. 

The  circular  profiles  have  some  compensating  virtues — they  are 
reliable,  they  are  quantitative,  and  they  are  standardized  and  cali- 
brated in  terms  of  the  sixteen  variables  by  which  we  measure  inter- 
personal behavior  at  other  levels.  This  means  that  we  can  directly 
compare  the  different  perceptions  which  the  patient  reports.  They 
allow  us  to  build  up  a  series  of  probability  laws  which  hold  for  each 
level  and  for  the  relationships  among  levels. 

Level  II  Presents  the  Patient's  View  of  the 
'^Transference''  Situation 
We  have  reviewed  how  a  patient  entering  the  Kaiser  Foundation 
Psychiatric  Clinic  diagnoses  himself  and  his  family  members.  The 
relationships  among  these  measures  have  provided  indices  of  conscious 
identification  and  conscious  equation.  When  the  intake  evaluation 
was  completed,  this  patient  began  psychotherapy.  After  nine  hours 
of  treatment  he  filled  out  the  Interpersonal  Check  List  on  his  therapist. 
This  gives  us  his  conscious  description  of  the  therapist  (Figure  11). 
The  patient  diagnoses  the  doctor. 


Figure  11.  Level  II-C  Description  of  Therapist  by  Patient 
Illustrating  a  Measure  of  (Conscious)  Transference.  Key:  Radius 
of  circle  equals  16  check  list  words. 

This  patient  reported  his  therapist  (Figure  11)  as  a  well-balanced 
person.  He  did  not  use  intense  or  one-sided  descriptive  terms.  He 
attributed  moderate  themes  of  strength  (A),  punitive  firmness  (D), 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION  143 

and  mild  rejection  (C)  to  his  therapist.  A  mild  negative  transference 
at  the  conscious  level  is  apparent. 

When  the  view  of  therapist  is  compared  with  his  pretreatment  view 
of  self,  we  see  that  the  patient  is  disidentified  with  the  therapist.  He 
reports  his  therapist  as  being  much  more  like  his  mother  and  his  wife 
(Figure  12). 

1    bitter  Mother      1    cold 

Self   I  and  ^-^     Wife  I  and 

J    depressed  Therapist  J    punitive 

In  this  manner  the  patient  gives  us  a  systematic  picture  of  his  oedi- 
pal  and  his  transference  situations— fro?;?  his  oivn  viewpoint.  The  pa- 
tient's reports  about  these  relationships  may  be  quite  different  from 
the  therapist's.  The  latter  might  not  consider  himself  as  being  cold 
and  strict  with  the  patient.  If  the  patient's  description  of  his  therapist 
is  consensually  inaccurate,  this  fact  takes  on  a  considerable  impor- 
tance in  understanding  the  treatment  relationship.  The  relation  be- 
tween the  patient's  view  of  another  and  the  consensual  view  of  that 
person  allows  for  an  operational  definition  of  a  classic  defense  mech- 
anism— projection.  Where  this  inaccurate  perception  involves  the 
therapist,  we  have  obtained  a  measure  of  transference-projection. 

Therapists  Can  Measure  Their  Own  Misperceptions 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  has  undertaken  extensive 
studies  of  process  in  psychotherapy.  The  aim  of  these  studies  is  to 
apply  the  interpersonal  system  to  the  therapeutic  interaction  and  to 
the  perceptions  of  the  patient  and  therapist.  The  working  principle 
employed  in  these  studies  is:  the  patient  and  therapist  comprise  a  basic 
interacting  unit.  We  do  not  study  the  patient  in  therapy,  but  both 
the  patient  and  the  therapist  as  they  interacted.  These  therapeutic 
studies  cannot  be  included  in  this  diagnostic  monograph,  but  they  are 
worth  brief  comment  here  because  they  illustrate  the  application  of 
Level  II  measurements. 

One  procedure  commonly  employed  is  to  have  the  therapist  fill  out 
an  Interpersonal  Check  List  on  his  patient.  This  gives  us  a  most  in- 
teresting measure.  It  tells  us  how  the  therapist  sees  the  patient  with 
whom  he  is  in  relationship.  Figure  1 2  presents  the  therapist's  picture 
of  the  patient  we  have  been  discussing.  The  circle  tells  us  that  the 
therapist  sees  the  patient  as  deferent  (/)  and  dependent  (/).  Now 
this  description  may  or  may  not  be  consensually  accurate.  Regard- 
less of  its  "pull,"  it  does  summarize  some  valuable  information — it  tells 
us  something  about  the  conscious  countertransference. 


J  .^  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

We  recall  that  the  patient  described  the  therapist  as  cold  and  re- 
jecting. This  implies  a  certain  fear  and  passive  hostility  on  the  part 
of  the  patient.  He  feels  mildly  rejected.  The  therapist  sees  the  pa- 
tient as  mainly  weak  and  dependent.  This  suggests  certain  discrepan- 
cies in  the  communication  pattern  between  the  two.  The  therapist 
might  fail  to  sense  the-  patient's  feelings  of  deprivation,  and  assume 
deference  and  collaboration. 


Figure  12.  Level  II-C  Description  of  Patient  by  His  Psycho- 
therapist Illustrating  a  Measure  of  (Conscious)  Countertransfer- 
ence.  Key:  Radius  of  circle  equals  16  check  list  words. 

Fitting  together  the  reciprocal  perceptions  by  both  members  of  the 
relationship  often  reveals  striking  breakdowns  in  communication. 
Projection  and  perceptual  distortion  on  the  part  of  patient  and  thera- 
pist often  become  apparent  by  the  use  of  Level  II  measures. - 

This  discussion  brings  us  to  a  tricky  problem  of  definition.  We 
have  been  talking  about  Level  II  behavior — the  conscious  descriptions 
of  self-and-other  by  the  patient.  When  we  introduced  the  therapist's 
view  of  the  patient  we  complicated  the  issues.  From  the  standpoint  of 
the  patient,  the  therapist's  view-of-patient  is  Level  I.  The  therapist 
is  thus  an  outsider  rating  the  patient. 

But  in  any  study  of  therapeutic  interaction  we  focus  equally  on 
patient  and  therapist.  The  therapist's  view  of  the  patient  is  a  Level 
II  Other  measure  from  the  standpoint  of  the  therapist.  Studies  in 
interpersonal  relations  which  attempt  to  use  multilevel  patterns  of 
response  can  become  quite  complex  since  we  must  study  both  sides  of 

^  A  most  ingenious  research  which  illustrates  the  phenomenon  of  countertrans- 
ference,  as  measured  by  the  interpersonal  system,  has  been  completed  by  Richard 
Cutler  (1).  This  research  deals  with  misperception  of  self  and  others  in  the  psycho- 
therapeutic simation. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION  145 

the  transaction.    A's  report  of  B  is  subjective  (Level  II)  from  the 
standpoint  of  A,  but  it  is  objective  (Level  I)  from  the  standpoint  of  B. 

The  specific  definition,  conceptuaHzation,  and  clinical  meaning  of 
these  relationship  variables  (e.g.,  identification,  projection)  need  not 
be  taken  up  in  detail  here.  In  the  context  of  this  discussion  of  Level  II 
behavior  it  need  only  be  suggested  that  the  relationships  between  self- 
perception  and  perception  of  "others"  have  considerable  importance. 
Lawful  connections  do  exist  between  these  self-and-other  circles. 
Patients  who  describe  themselves  as  distrustful  and  isolated  tend  to 
present  predictable  pictures  of  their  parents  and  the  significant  "oth- 
ers" in  their  lives.  So  do  the  patients  who  assign  themselves  to  other 
extreme  positions  on  the  Level  II  circle. 

A  host  of  low-order  predictive  functions  can  be  called  into  play 
if  we  obtain  this  one  type  of  personality  measure — a  patient's  con- 
scious description  of  self.  It  should  be  kept  clear  that  these  are  not 
foolproof  prognostications.  They  are  probability  statements  which 
allow  us  to  make  such  predictions  as,  "If  the  patient  describes  himself 
as  siveet  and  docile  (/K),  the  chances  are  5  to  1  he  will  attribute  to 
at  least  one  parent  ideahzed,  tender  nurturance,  and  the  chances  are 
2  to  1  that  he  will  see  both  parents  in  this  way."  From  Level  II  Self- 
description  alone  we  obtain  a  large  but  loose  network  of  low-order 
probability  statements  which  make  predictions  about  other  levels  and 
areas.  This  is  interesting  theoretically,  but  of  restricted  practical  value. 
When  we  add  the  data  from  another  level  or  area — his  view  of  parents, 
for  example — the  additional  evidence  tightens  up  the  network  of  re- 
lationships. It  increases  the  complexity  of  the  personality  structure  in 
a  geometrical  rather  than  an  arithmetical  proportion.  That  is,  it  multi- 
plies the  permutations  and  combinations  of  relationships.  It  also  in- 
creases the  accuracy  of  prediction. 

Use  of  Level  II  Patterns  in  Child  Guidance 

We  have  stressed  the  point  that  Level  II  conscious  descriptions  are 
the  most  simple,  straightforward  measures  of  personality  and  their 
maximum  usefulness  is  found  in  combining  or  comparing  these  con- 
scious reports  with  other  levels. 

In  child  guidance.  Level  II  patterns  seem  to  have  an  especially 
valuable  application.  Diagnostic  evaluation  of  children's  cases  is  a 
complex  process.  One  of  the  difficult  aspects  of  this  procedure  is 
caused  by  the  multiplicity  of  interpersonal  relations  involved.  Under- 
standing the  child's  situation  requires  some  knowledge  of  the  child,  his 
siblings,  both  parents.  Often  the  parents'  attitudes  toward  child-rear- 
ing are  closely  related  to  their  own  parents.  Thus,  a  three-generation 
matrix  of  relationships  can  be  involved. 


Mother's  view  of: 


MATERNAL   GRANDMOTHER 


MATERNAL   GRANDFATHER 

lAP) 


(HI) 

CHILD    BY    MOTHER 


Figure  13.    Familial  Pattern  of  Interpersonal 
146 


Father's  view  of: 


PATERNAL   GRANDFATHER 

■f4p. 


PATERNAL   GRANDMOTHER 


S  (H  . 
FATHER   BY    WIFE 


(hTT 
FATHER    BY   SELF 


•(h7)~ 
CHILD  BY   FATHER 
Relations  for  Child  Guidance  Evaluation. 


H7 


148  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

A  convenient  way  to  systematize  these  patterns  is  to  obtain  Level 
II-C  descriptions  from  both  parents.  The  mother,  for  example,  is 
asked  to  rate  herself,  her  husband,  her  child,  and  both  of  her  parents. 
The  husband  does  the  same.  As  indicated  in  Figure  1 3  a  matrix  of  ten 
ratings  is  obtained.  This  three-generation  pattern  of  interpersonal 
scores  provides  a  large  number  of  cues  for  understanding  the  child 
and  his  emotional  background. 

The  family  constellation  diagrammed  in  Figure  1 3  reveals  that  the 
mother  sees  herself  as  responsible,  generous,  and  hypernormal;  that  she 
sees  her  child  as  rebellious  and  passive-resistant,  her  husband  as  being 
like  the  child.  She  describes  her  mother  as  being  a  strong,  responsible 
figure  and  her  father  as  being  a  bitter  and  defeated  man.  The  father, 
on  the  other  hand,  describes  the  child  as  being  less  rebellious — which 
suggests  that  most  of  the  friction  (and  perhaps  the  motivating  force 
in  bringing  the  child  to  the  clinic)  centers  around  the  mother.  The 
father  tends  to  see  himself  like  the  child  and  like  his  own  father.  He 
describes  his  wife  as  being  very  much  like  his  mother.  A  fairly  wide 
discrepancy  exists  between  the  wife's  self-description  and  her  hus- 
band's view  of  her — he  attributes  much  more  hostility  and  bossiness 
to  her  than  she  admits.  This  interwoven  pattern  of  mutual  mispercep- 
tion  and  oedipal  themes  suggests  several  hypotheses  which  may  be 
useful  in  understanding  this  family's  situation  and  the  problems  which 
led  the  parents  to  bring  the  child  to  the  clinic. 

According  to  Mary  Sarvis  (2),  it  is  not  unusual  to  find  that  the  par- 
ents have  quite  different  perceptions  of  the  child,  or  that  a  certain 
personality  formation  characteristic  of  a  grandparent  has  skipped  a 
generation  and  reappeared  in  the  child. 

When  these  scores  are  compared  with  each  other,  a  complex  net- 
work of  discrepancy  indices  can  be  derived.  We  can  measure  the  dif- 
ference between  the  husband's  view  of  himself  and  his  wife's  percep- 
tion of  him;  or  the  similarity  between  the  wife's  view  of  her  father, 
her  own  husband,  and  the  child.  These  relationships  will  be  given 
operational  definition  and  further  theoretical  consideration  in  Chapter 
13,  which  deals  with  the  measurement  of  variability  indices. 

Variability  and  the  Sublevels  of  Consciousness 

At  the  end  of  the  last  chapter,  it  became  necessary  to  consider  the 
objection  that  our  clinical  measurements  are  infinitesimally  narrow 
contrasted  with  the  broad  variety  of  human  behavior.  In  each  differ- 
ent situation  and  at  every  point  in  time  we  deal  with  a  changing  or- 
ganism. Our  measurements,  however  elaborate,  are  generally  limited 
to  a  restricted  range  and  to  a  fleeting  span  of  time.  It  has  been  sug- 
gested that  the  predictions  be  limited  to  the  context  in  which  the 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


149 


measurements  are  made.  The  Kaiser  Foundation  system  is  a  func- 
tional clinical  system.  By  aiming  our  predictions  at  the  further  be- 
havior of  the  patient  in  the  clinic,  we  use  our  information  in  the  same 
context  in  which  it  was  collected. 

Now,  as  we  come  to  the  close  of  this  chapter,  we  are  faced  again 
with  the  same  issue.  The  range  of  conscious  reports  is  diverse.  A  per- 
son describes  himself  in  a  variety  of  ways,  depending  on  his  purposes 
and  the  environmental,  situation.  He  will  emphasize  certain  trends 
when  he  attempts  to  impress,  others  when  he  attempts  to  excuse  him- 
self, others  when  motivated  to  confide.  How  do  we  know  that  the 
measurements  of  Level  II  we  make  in  one  or  even  several  moments  of 
time  reflect  the  over-all  scope  of  the  individual's  conscious  percep- 
tions? Of  course,  we  don't  know.  Here  we  must  resign  ourselves  to 
the  familiar  indeterminism.  We  can  never  hope  to  sample  the  breadth 
of  the  individual's  self-descriptions  as  they  vary  in  time  and  context. 

There  are,  fortunately,  several  steps  which  can  be  taken  to  limit  our 
ignorance.  The  first  of  these  derives  from  the  organismic  premise  that 
no  datum  of  personality  can  be  evaluated  except  in  the  context  of  the 
total  organization.  The  tenor  of  our  conscious  reports  is,  as  we  well 
know,  related  to  the  Level  I  situation.  What  one  says  depends  upon 
what  one  purposes.  It  depends  upon  the  pressure  of  the  social  en- 
vironment. Let  us  illustrate.  Consider  an  initial  treatment  interview 
in  which  the  therapist  is  reflexly  and  unconsciously  pushing  the  pa- 
tient to  free-associate  and  confide.  Let  us  assume  that  the  interview 
has  been  recorded  and  the  independent  judge  rates  this  behavior  as 
directive  (AP  at  Level  I  Other)  behavior.  Let  the  patient  be  B  (stub- 
born, resistive) .  We  shall  skip  the  question  of  responsibility  for  initiat- 
ing the  relationship,  i.e.,  who  provoked  whom  to  develop  these  roles. 
In  most  relationships  this  is  a  mutual  process  of  training  each  other. 
In  this  context,  the  therapist  might  make  a  brief  didactic  remark  to  the 
effect  that  people  sometimes  have  feelings  about  their  childhood  or 
about  their  parents  that  are  important.  The  patient  might  produce  the 
conscious  description  of  self  and  other  that  she  has  nothing  but  the 
most  loving  feelings  toward  her  parents  who  have  always  been  kind 
and  good  to  her.  The  following  oversimplified  formula  has  developed: 

Self  (Patient)  Other 

Level  I  B  < — >  AP  (therapist) 

Level  II        M  < — >  O  (parents) 

We  verbally  summarize  by  saying:  "When  the  patient  is  being 
resistive  or  defensive  to  a  directive  therapist,  she  reports  herself  as  af- 
fectionate to  her  idealized  parents.   Let  us  go  on  to  assume  that  the 


1^0  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

therapist  works  through  this  power  struggle  and  that  by  the  twentieth 
treatment  interview  a  participant  phase  of  the  relationship  is  develop- 
ing. The  therapist  is  communicating  support  (N),  and  the  patient  is 
attempting  to  be  cooperative  (L),  In  this  context  she  might  confide, 
"Many  times  I  have  been  disappointed  and  hurt  by  my  parents'  un- 
willingness to  understand  me  and  my  point  of  view,"  The  formula 
for  this  sequence  becomes: 

Self  Other 

Level  I  L  < — >  N  (therapist) 

Level  II        G  < — >  C   (parents) 

This  translates  back  to  the  verbal  summary:  When  the  patient  is 
confiding  and  cooperative  to  a  supportive  therapist,  she  describes  her- 
self as  rejected  by  her  parents  who  are  seen  as  unsympathetic. 

Here  the  content  of  conscious  report  has  shifted  dramatically  in  re- 
lation to  the  interpersonal  purposes  involved.  If,  however,  we  had 
drawn  hard  and  fast  diagnostic  conclusions  after  the  first  interview,  a 
most  incomplete  picture  would  have  developed,  A  working  rule  thus 
develops.  The  data  of  conscious  report  must  be  studied  in  the  light  of 
the  three  standard  sources  of  variation:  time,  the  interpersonal  context, 
and  variation  among  the  levels  of  personality.  The  last,  which  defines 
structural  variation,  involves  the  relationship  between  the  levels  of 
self-behavior.  In  the  illustration  we  have  just  considered,  we  focus  on 
the  interpersonal  context  in  which  the  parents  were  described.  The 
patient's  Level  I  purposes  shifted  from  defensive  disagreement  to  co- 
operation, as  the  social  environment,  in  the  form  of  the  therapist, 
shifted  its  directive  pressure. 

It  follows  that  one  control  over  the  variability  in  conscious  descrip- 
tion is  obtained  by  indicating  the  organism — world  matrix  from  which 
the  data  come.  Since  patients  give  us  their  views  of  self-world  in  the 
context  in  which  we  wish  to  employ  the  data — i.e.,  in  the  clinic — the 
functional  criterion  enters  again  as  a  second  useful  control  over  varia- 
bility. By  pointing  our  predictions  to  future  behavior  in  the  clinic, 
we  keep  constant,  or  at  least  more  constant,  the  situational  factor.  The 
advantage  of  limiting  our  predictions  to  the  functional  nexus  has  al- 
ready been  considered  in  the  preceding  chapter.  They  are  equally 
applicable  to  the  problem  of  Level  II  variation, 

A  third  partial  solution  to  the  issue  of  variation  involves  technical 
procedures  in  the  collection  of  data.  There  is  a  wide  variety  of  meth- 
ods for  obtaining  Level  II  material  in  the  clinical  situation — interview, 
check  list,  autobiography,  etc.  They  range  from  the  personal  revela- 
tion at  the  most  intensive  moments  of  psychotherapy  to  mechanical  se- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


151 


lection  of  "yes"  or  "no"  items  on  a  questionnaire.  For  a  complete  eval- 
uation of  personality,  we  optimally  obtain  as  many  different  types  of 
self-report  as  possible — as  the  subject  varies  in  response  to  the  most 
free  through  the  most  controlled  stimuli,  from  the  most  confiding  to 
the  most  defensive  motivations. 

In  this  w2Ly  we  tap  not  just  the  patient's  self-description  as  revealed 
by  one  Level  II  measure — but  rather  a  range  of  Level  II  behaviors.  If 
the  same  self-description  emerges  from  all  the  measures,  then  we  can 
be  fairly  certain  that  we  have  a  durable  estimate  of  Level  II.  If  it 
varies  among  the  different  sublevel  measures,  then  we  have  an  esti- 
mate of  the  changeability  of  the  self-description  and  the  way  it  varies. 

Figure  14  provides  a  hypothetical  Level  II  variation  in  depth  of  con- 
scious reports  in  different  cultural  contexts.  The  problem  of  varia- 
bility, which  we  have  raised  here,  is  discussed  in  detail  in  a  later  sec- 
tion of  this  book. 


Various  Level  II  Measures 


Level  III 


Level  II-C         Level  II-C  Level  Il-Ti        Level  II-Ti 


Check  list 

of  self 
during  job 
application 


o 


Check  list 

of  self 
when  tested 
in  clinic 


o 


De<:cription 
of  self 
at  begin- 
ning of 
therapy 


Description 

of  self 

after  one 

year  of 

therapy 


o 


o 


Figure  14.  Hypothetical  Variation  in  Depth  of  Level  II  Measurements  Due  to 
Change  in  Cultural  Context. 

Incidence  of  Level  ll-C  Behavior  in  Various  Cultural  Samples 

Summaries  of  research  findings  involving  Level  II  behavior  are  lo- 
cated in  the  pertinent  clinical  chapters  to  follow.  To  familiarize  the 
reader  with  some  of  the  general  meaning  of  conscious  descriptions  of 
self  and  others  the  percentage  of  Level  II  types  occurring  in  several 
symptomatic  samples  will  now  be  presented  in  Table  3. 


^2  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


s  ss§ 


lEuuottiadXH 

o 

-ajqtsuodsa-a 

:^ 

S 

IBUOI1U3AUO0J3AO 

:^ 

g 

-3AUBJ3(i003 

>-J 

« 

CD 

"S 

juapuadaQ 

.J 
< 

5 

-aipoQ 

U 
g 

1 

» 

Q 

DUStqoosEi/V 

5 

u 

§ 

-2m3Zi}2-iPS 

■« 
a 

1 

£ 

o 

jnjisaiastQ 

^ 

CO 

%> 

-snoiipqan 

t^ 

z 

^ 

— 

s 

o 

R 

z 

?J 

DUSipEg 

uj 

1 

t 

-3AISS3j3Sv 

Q 

Uh 

Q, 

^»^ 

1 

G 

ansissiojEfsj 

O 

ca 

K 

-3AUU0duiO3 

03 

< 

>J 

h 

^ 

DUEJDoanv 

Q- 

-JEUaSBUEI^ 

^ 

l/^   NO  -^        rr,   ^ 


O^  O  OO       O  Tj-  O  r-j 


Ov  vo   ■^  — 


«   00        o   ^   -^ 


r~~  —      vo  o  -^  o 


"^       "  .ii  E 

^^       c  '5  ■«  ■" 

!2   «   w  c£   S  t** 

o  2  -H  Oh  -a  S  5  g 

'S  «  ji  'w  p^  -n  „             ^ 


-7^  n  X  (1  H  -s 


THE  LEVEL  OF  CONSCIOUS  COMMUNICATION 


153 


It  will  be  noted  that  in  their  self-diagnosis  individual  psychotherapy 
patients  emphasize  passivity  (HI),  group  therapy  patients  distrust 
(FG),  ulcer  patients  aggressiveness  (DE),  hypertensive  and  obese  pa- 
tients hypernormal  strength  (AP  and  NO). 

Tests  of  significance  among  these  samples  and  a  detailed  discussion 
of  the  implications  of  these  findings  will  be  presented  in  Chapter  24. 

References 

1.  Cutler,  R.  The  relationship  between  the  therapist's  personality  and  certain  aspects 
of  psychotherapy.  Unpubhshed  doctor's  dissertation,  University  of  Michigan, 
1954. 

2.  Sarvis,  Mary.  Personal  communication. 


The  Level  of  Private  Perception. 
The  Interpersonal  Symbol 


The  third  level  of  personality — Level  III — comprises  the  expressions 
that  an  individual  makes,  not  directly  about  his  real  self  in  his  real 
world,  but  indirectly  about  an  imagined  self  in  his  preconscious  or 
symbolic  world.  The  interpersonal  motives  and  actions  attributed  to 
the  figures  who  people  his  fantasies,  his  creative  expressions,  his  wishes, 
his  dreams  define  the  subject  matter  for  this  level  of  personality.  They 
are  called  preconscious  symbolic  expressions  because  they  stand  for 
or  symbolize  aspects  of  the  subject  (and  his  world)  which  are  not  di- 
rectly denoted.  The  subject  selects  and  employs  themes.  But  he  at- 
tributes them  not  to  himself  or  to  his  real  world,  but  in  an  imaginary 
context.  They  do  have  a  relationship — although  indirect  and  often  un- 
witting— to  his  conscious  and  communicative  behavior.  They  have  an 
expressive  function,  not  direct,  but  symbolic. 

The  use  of  the  term  preconscious  to  describe  Level  III  expressions 
is  a  debatable  procedure.  The  preconscious  as  defined  operationally 
in  this  volume  cannot  be  equated  with  the  term  as  used  by  psycho- 
analysts. Kris  (6,  p.  542)  has  cited  two  quotations  from  Freud  which 
define  the  preconscious.  "In  defining  the  quality  of  the  preconscious, 
Freud  follows  Breuer:  preconscious  is  what  is  'capable  of  becoming 
conscious,'  and  he  adds,  'capable  of  becoming  conscious  easily  and 
under  conditions  which  frequently  arise.'  "  It  might  seem,  at  first 
glance,  that  the  symbolic  and  projective  responses  which  define  Level 
III  might  meet  Freud's  definition.  The  empirical  situation  is,  un- 
happily, not  that  simple.  In  actuality  subjects  do  not  always  express 
in  response  to  projective  stimuli  their  private  or  fantasy  thoughts. 
Many  defensive,  suppressive,  rigid  patients  repeat  in  their  response  to 
projective  stimuli  the  same  themes  they  report  in  their  conscious  de- 
scriptions. What  we  get  at  Level  III  is,  therefore,  not  preconscious  ma- 

154 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  155 

terial,  but  those  themes  which  the  subject  is  wilUng  to  express  in  the 
testing  situation.  Level  III  is  defined  by  the  source  of  the  data.  Level 
III  behavior  should,  therefore,  be  accurately  labeled  as  the  "response 
to  projective  stimuU."  This  may  not  be  indirect  or  symbolic  or  pre- 
conscious.  Since  there  is  no  single  term  in  the  English  language  for 
denoting  "that  which  the  subject  chooses  to  express  in  reaction  to  pro- 
jective stimuli,"  I  have  hesitantly  employed  the  familiar  terms  "sym- 
bolic, imaginative,  indirect,  fantasy,  projective  and  preconscious"  as 
synonyms  for  Level  III  behavior.  To  remind  the  reader  that  the 
psychoanalytic  concept  is  not  denoted,  the  word  preconscious  will  be 
consistently  in  quotes. 

Whenever  the  subject  shifts  the  content  of  expression  from  the 
actuality — believed,  perceived,  described — to  the  imagined,  fantasied, 
then  he  is  communicating  in  the  symbolic  mode.  In  practice  this  dis- 
tinction is  quite  simple  and  straightforward.  The  content  of  dreams, 
fantasies,  creative  expressions,  wishes,  projective  tests,  automatically 
becomes  Level  III  data. 

The  Paradox  of  Symbolic  Life 

The  phenomenon  of  symbolization  is  one  of  the  most  puzzling  as- 
pects of  human  behavior.  In  the  first  place  it  seems  to  be  a  universal 
phenomenon.  The  dream,  that  obscure,  enigmatic  ripple  across  the 
surface  of  rational  life,  is  shared  by  the  most  literate  and  the  most 
primitive  mind.  Rituals,  legends,  myths,  fantasies  are  woven  into  the 
histories  of  all  people  and  all  cultures. 

A  second  paradoxical  quality  of  symbols  is  their  function.  Why  do 
all  men  channel  so  much  energy  into  symbolization?  Man's  response 
to  the  physical  elements  is  fairly  well  rationalized.  We  can  explain 
the  cognitive  aspects  of  behavior — the  communicative  functions  of 
sign,  gesture,  words  in  the  pattern  of  social  survival  has  been  exten- 
sively studied.  While  the  theories  disagree  in  detail,  the  general 
purpose  of  representative  signs,  whether  cries  of  alarm  or  notational 
ciphers,  is  an  exphcable  area  of  knowledge.  Genetic  and  evolutionary 
theories  have  had  considerable  success  in  explaining  the  survival  value 
of  these  communications.  The  discursive  language  of  factual  descrip- 
tion, which  we  have  discussed  in  the  last  chapter,  is  the  most  elaborated 
and  practical  aspect  of  human  intellectual  life.  The  essence  of  this 
form  of  expression  is  that  it  refers  to  events  and  things  in  the  objective 
world.  This  is  called  the  representational  function. 

The  symbolic  function  is,  however,  quite  a  different  phenomenon. 
It  does  not  refer  to  the  world  as  it  is  seen  by  others.  It  does  not  neces- 
sarily rely  on  the  lawful  principles  that  regulate  the  events  of  reality. 
Miraculous,  magical  processes  can  transpire  in  fantasies.  The  limits  of 


,j(5  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

space  and  time  do  not  apply  in  dreams.  The  unreal  quality  of  symbolic 
productions,  which  for  some  philosophers  is  its  outstanding  character- 
istic, certainly  assigns  them  a  different  function  in  the  economy  of 
human  life.  The  imaginative  mode  is  of  little  direct  use  in  dealing  im- 
mediately with  the  practical  aspects  of  life.  Thus,  as  Langer  points 
out,  creative-autistic  expressions  cannot  be  explained  in  terms  of  their 
survival  value  in  dealing  with  the  real  world.  In  many  cases  man's 
myths  and  fantastic  autisms  have  confused  and  hampered  his  adjust- 
ment to  the  environment  around  him  (7). 

The  most  persuasive  solution  to  this  paradox  is  that  symbolic  ex- 
pression is  not  a  response  by  which  man  deals  with  the  challenging 
stimuli  of  the  external  environment;  it  is  a  response  to  internal  am- 
biguity and  tension. 

Reversal  Theory  of  Symbols 

In  Chapter  7  when  we  discussed  the  level  of  public  communication, 
considerable  emphasis  was  placed  on  the  reflex  tendency  to  select  cer- 
tain interpersonal  responses  and  to  avoid  others.  The  phenomenon  of 
reciprocal  interpersonal  relations  formalized  this  automatic  process  by 
which  we  pull  certain  reactions  from  others  and,  in  turn,  respond  with 
a  limited  set  of  behaviors.  The  stable  continuity  that  thus  develops 
results  in  an  imbalance.  Certain  interpersonal  techniques  for  minimiz- 
ing anxiety  are  automatically  employed.  Others  which  cause  anxiety 
are  less  favored. 

In  Jungian  terms,  certain  interpersonal  functions  are  overdeveloped; 
others  are  neglected.  One  side  of  the  circle  is  predominant,  the  other 
inhibited.  An  imbalance  at  one  level  of  personality  can  have  many 
possible  relationships  to  the  rest  of  the  character  structure.  The  sim- 
plest and  most  classic  case  is  the  reversal  concept  repression  of  the  op- 
posite. Here  symbols  are  held  to  express  the  exact  opposite  of  overt  or 
conscious  behavior.  This  is  an  appealing  solution  of  the  conscious- 
unconscious  problem  and  has  by  far  the  most  common  sense  appeal. 
According  to  this  version,  something  like  a  conservation  of  energy 
process  is  at  work.  The  themes  which  are  inhibited  and  denied  from 
overt  manifestation  are  held  to  be  expressed  in  symbolic  life.  Almost 
every  theorist  who  has  written  on  the  psychology  of  symbolism  has 
leaned  on  this  notion.  Much  anecdotal  evidence  supports  it.  The 
private  life  of  Walter  Mitty  is  shot  through  with  the  acclaim,  success, 
and  mastery  which  he  does  not  express  in  his  prosaic  life. 

Objections  to  the  Reversal  Theory  of  Symbols 

The  general  popularity  of  the  reversal  theory  of  symbolism  has  not 
been  diminished  by  the  two  demurrers  that  can  be  raised  against  it.  In 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  157 

the  first  place,  there  are  many  cases  in  which  it  just  does  not  work — 
the  nightmare  dream,  to  take  the  extreme  example,  in  which  the  pain 
of  reality  is  repeated  again  and  again  in  exaggerated  form.  A  second 
restraint  on  the  easy  acceptance  of  this  theory  is  that  it  has  never  had 
objective  assessment.  The  repression  of  the  opposite  theory  has  never 
been  put  to  the  test  because  such  testing  requires  a  systematic  method 
for  measuring  behavior  at  the  conscious  and  overt  levels,  for  measuring 
behavior  at  the  symbolic  level  in  terms  of  the  same  variable  continuum, 
and  for  relating  the  different  levels. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychology  Research  Group  has  attempted 
to  test  this  reversal  hypothesis  in  a  series  of  correlational  studies.  This 
research  suggests  that  the  tendency  for  symbols  to  express  the  opposite 
of  conscious  or  public  behavior  is  not  universal  or  inevitable.  It  holds 
for  about  half  of  our  cases,  some  of  the  time.  Some  persons  do  tend 
to  employ  symbols  which  are  the  opposite  of  their  conscious  and  pub- 
lic imbalances,  but  others  tend  to  report  monotonously  in  their  sym- 
bols the  same  themes  which  characterize  their  behavior  at  other  levels. 
Thus,  the  reversal  or  equilibrium  theory  of  fantasy  is  not  a  general 
finding.  It  varies  from  person  to  person.  This  variability,  the  tendency 
to  use  symbols  which  are  the  same  or  different  from  consciousness,  is 
a  measurable,  stable,  psychological  variable.  In  Chapter  13  it  will  be 
defined  and  validated  as  a  separate  and  vital  dimension  of  personality 
in  its  own  right. 

Symbols  Are  Important  in  Relationship  to  Other  Levels 

The  fallacy  behind  oversimplified  hypotheses  such  as  the  reversal  of 
sy^nbols  theory  is  that  they  focus  on  a  single  level  of  personality — at 
best,  two  levels — and  attempt  to  generalize  laws.  The  results  are  bound 
to  be  disappointing  and  misleading. 

We  approach  the  indirect  imaginative  productions  of  the  human 
being  not  expecting  them  to  serve  any  single  function.  Any  level  takes 
on  its  full  meaning  only  in  relationship  to  all  the  other  levels,  that  is, 
to  the  total  personality  organization.  This  is  the  organismic  assump- 
tion. Every  level  or  area  of  personality  is  in  dynamic  equilibrium  with 
all  the  other  levels  and  the  total  intricate  system  of  balance  and 
counterbalance  makes  up  the  fabulous  complexity  we  call  personality. 
To  prevent  this  organismic  assumption  from  becoming  a  truism,  the 
syntactical  procedures  determining  the  exact  number  of  the  interlevel 
relationships  must  be  made  explicit,  then  the  connection  between 
Level  III  and  the  other  levels  of  personality  can  be  defined,  measured, 
validated,  and  understood.  The  permutations  and  combinations  of 
levels  according  to  the  present  notational  system  will  be  presented 
in  Chapter  13. 


1^8  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

Some  Misconceptions  About  Projective  Tests 

These  findings  carry  along  in  their  wake  some  implications  for  the 
projective  testing  movement.  This  branch  of  clinical  psychology 
concerns  itself  with  responses  to  stimulus  items  which  are  unstructured 
or  semistructured.  The  subject  is  given  vague  or  incomplete  test  cards 
and  asked  to  give  his  interpretation  of  them.  In  the  Rorschach  test  he 
tells  what  he  "sees"  in  vague  inkblot  shapes;  in  the  Thematic  Apper- 
ception Test  (TAT)  he  tells  stories  which  he  believes  fit  and  com- 
plete the  actions  portrayed  in  magazine-type  illustrations.  The  essence 
of  the  technique  is  that  the  stimuli  are  ambiguous  to  some  degree  and 
the  patient  "projects"  his  own  imaginative  perceptions.  He  attributed 
his  own  fantasy  themes.  The  theory  claims  that  through  his  symbols 
the  subject  shall  be  known.  The  themes  elicited  are  believed  to  re- 
flect a  "deeper"  and  more  valid  picture  of  his  personality  than  those 
of  conscious  report. 

The  field  of  projective  testing  is  a  theoretical  shambles.  In  the  first 
place,  interpersonal,  Freudian,  Jungian,  and  stimulus-bound  variables 
are  jumbled  together.  The  diagnosis  is  often  made  in  a  rag-tail  man- 
ner, stressing  whatever  variables  happen  to  drift  into  focus  in  the 
patient's  responses  or  in  the  clinician's  observations.  There  is  rarely 
any  attempt  to  separate  levels.  For  these  reasons,  most  of  the  objec- 
tive assessments  of  projective  test  practices  have  come  up  with  nega- 
tive findings.  Almost  every  time  that  independent  researchers  have 
tested  the  hypotheses  and  predictions  involved  in  projective  testing  to 
see  if  they  really  work,  the  answer  is  "no."  It  can  be  flatly  said  that 
the  field  of  projective  testing,  whatever  its  popularity,  is  an  unvalidated 
or  unsatisfactorily  validated  enterprise. 

The  great  potential  value  of  this  approach  to  the  symbolic  has  born 
little  fruit  because  the  systematic  conceptualization  of  levels  and 
variables  of  personality  has  not  been  employed.  Consider,  for  example, 
the  patient  who  produces  fantasy  materials  which  are  saturated  with 
themes  of  bitterness  and  murderous  anger.  What  can  we  say  about  the 
person  on  the  basis  of  these  data?  Not  very  much.  We  know  that  his 
symbols  are  hostile,  but  without  knowledge  of  the  other  levels,  our 
predictions  are  very  limited.  If  this  subject  consciously  describes  him- 
self as  loving-agreeable,  the  symbolic  rage  takes  on  one  significance. 
If  the  bitterness  assigned  to  his  fantasy  heroes  is  also  attributed  to  him- 
self at  Level  II,  quite  a  different  interpretation  results. 

Formal,  Noninterpersonal  Aspects  of  Projective  Tests 

In  addition  to  the  content,  another  aspect  of  projective  tests  refers 
to  the  so-called  formal  qualities.  Here  the  clinician  studies  not  the 
themes  but  the  expressive  and  stylistic  factors  of  the  subject's  response. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


59 


The  impulsivity,  constriction,  obsessive  deliberation,  flexibility,  care- 
lessness of  the  performance  are  observed  and  measured.  They  are  then 
indicated  in  the  diagnostic  report  as  characteristic  traits. 

These  behaviors  comprise  a  valid  and  important  aspect  of  personal- 
ity. They  are  noninterpersonal,  i.e.,  they  refer  to  symptomatic,  stylis- 
tic mood  factors.  They  shift  us  into  a  dimension  of  personality  which 
is  distinct  from  (although  lawfully  related  to)  interpersonal  behavior. 
Two  critical  comments  appear  to  be  appropriate  in  considering  these 
noninterpersonal  variables.  They  are  an  important  part  of  diagnostic 
procedure.  However,  they  take  on  increased  meaning  to  the  extent 
that  they  are  systematically  related  to  interpersonal  variables.  No  test 
report  is  complete  which  summarizes  the  noninterpersonal  style  and 
mood  aspects  of  behavior  and  fails  to  include  interpersonal  prediction. 
Noninterpersonal  variables  like  any  other  personality  measurements 
have  meaning  only  in  relationship  to  the  total  multilevel  pattern  of 
purposive  behavior.  The  statement:  "The  patient  acts  depressed,  im- 
mobilized, and  constricted  on  the  Rorschach"  is  a  good  diagnostic  be- 
ginning, but  it  takes  on  considerably  more  meaning  when  we  fit  it  into 
the  broader  purposive  context,  "His  immobilized  sadness  is  accom- 
panied by  self-descriptions  of  weakness  and  helplessness  (Level  II) 
and  by  dependent  pressure  directed  toward  the  clinician  (Level  I), 
etc.,  etc." 

The  symptomatic,  diagnostic  conditions  which  are  not  directly 
interpersonal  have  been  traditionally  the  central  concern  of  descrip- 
tive or  medically  oriented  psychiatry.  The  Kraepelinian  clinician  is 
especially  interested  in  the  peripheral  area  of  mood  (depressed,  agi- 
tated, manic)  or  style  of  expression  (bizarre,  obsessive,  disorganized, 
impulsive) .  The  more  physiological-neurological  the  psychiatrist's  ap- 
proach, the  more  you  may  be  sure  that  he  will  avoid  interpersonal 
terminology  and  depend  on  the  peripheral-symptomatic.  In  shifting 
the  emphasis  to  the  social  dimensions  of  personality,  we  by  no  means 
neglect  the  noninterpersonal.  We  make  the  hypothesis  that  the  expres- 
sive and  mood  variables  of  personality  have  a  basic,  although  indirect 
meaning.  As  we  shall  see,  they  are  related  significantly  to  interpersonal 
factors  at  different  levels  of  personality.  The  symptomatic  aspects 
of  psychiatry  thus  take  on  an  interpersonal  meaning.  They  are  related 
to  interpersonal  purposes.   They  predict  interpersonal  behavior. 

A  second  comment  can  be  made  in  regard  to  the  noninterpersonal 
variables  tapped  by  some  projective  tests.  It  should  be,  but  rarely  is, 
kept  clear  that  these  reactions  have  little  or  nothing  to  do  with  the 
symbolic  mode.  They  comprise  an  entirely  separate  dimension  of  be- 
havior. Certain  expressive  noninterpersonal  factors  are  related  to 
specific  interpersonal  themes  (e.g.,  energeticness  is  related  to  assertive- 


,^o  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

ness),  but  two  discrete  dimensions  of  measurement  are  involved.  Now 
the  content  of  imaginative  expressions — the  themes  and  purposive 
motifs — comprise  the  language  of  symbolism  which  we  study  as  Level 
III  data.  The  manner  in  which  the  subject  deals  with  the  symbolic 
stimulus  materials — lethargically,  constrictedly,  unhappily — define  an- 
other level  and  dimension  of  personality.  The  subject  can  produce  a 
fantasy  story  in  which  the  themes  involve  dashing,  daring,  careless 
impulsivity;  but  the  way  in  which  he  narrates  his  story  can  be  de- 
liberate, plodding,  and  painstaking.  The  theme  of  the  story  can  be 
quite  different  from  the  way  in  which  it  is  told.  In  this  case,  the  inter- 
personal content  of  imaginative  expressions  is  assigned  to  Level  III 
while  ratings  of  the  manner  and  style  of  expression  are  assigned  to 
Level  I  in  the  noninterpersonal  dimension,  since  they  are  actions  ob- 
served and  judged  by  others. 

Confusion  and  vagueness  about  levels,  failure  to  define  them,  and 
neglect  of  logical  systematization  has  led  to  this  strange  situation: 
many  projective  tests  are  employed  to  tap  and  study  not  the  symbolic 
mode,  but  the  motor,  perceptual  response  of  the  subject.  The  exceed- 
ingly popular  Rorschach  test  stands  out  as  the  classic  example  in  this 
regard.  The  standard  text  on  Rorschach  analysis  devotes  over  seventy 
per  cent  of  its  interpretative  attention  to  variables  that  have  nothing  to 
do  with  the  symbolic  mode.  The  subject's  perceptual,  executive,  or- 
ganizational techniques,  the  freedom  or  constriction  of  his  "affect," 
the  accuracy  or  deviation  of  his  perceptions  and  similar  topics  carry 
the  interpretative  burden.  The  nature  and  meaning  of  the  symbolic 
language — the  content  of  the  responses — has  generally  been  the  step- 
child of  Rorschach-type  theory. 

The  importance  of  the  perceptual  and  motor  executive  aspects  of 
behavior  should,  emphatically,  not  be  minimized.  To  measure  these 
Level  I  noninterpersonal  variables  many  straightforward  testing  tech- 
niques suggest  themselves.  Experimental  psychology  and  aptitude 
testing  procedures  give  any  number  of  techniques  for  assessing  im- 
pulsivity, organizational  synthesizing  abilities,  perceptual  and  intel- 
lectual functioning.  All  of  these  techniques  are  free  from  the  ex- 
'  haustive  stimulus-bound  complexity  of  the  Rorschach.  They  are  also 
free  from  the  crystal  ball,  medicine-man  aura  of  the  ink  blot  pro- 
cedure, which  often  lends  a  mysterious  and  untherapeutic  tone  to  the 
clinical  contact. 

To  measure  symbolic  behavior  it  is  necessary  to  focus  on  the  the- 
matic aspect  of  the  imaginative  production.  The  diminishing  popu- 
larity of  the  Rorschach  and  the  increasing  trend  toward  content 
analysis  in  projective  tests  are,  from  this  standpoint,  healthy  develop- 
ments. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  i6i 

Formal  Versus  Thematic  Interpretation  of  Symbols 

This  distinction  between  form  of  expression  and  content  of  expres- 
sion has  been  made  by  most  psychologists  who  have  concerned  them- 
selves with  thematic  tests.  (II)  The  syntax  of  levels  sharpens  this  di- 
vision and  takes  the  important  step  of  assigning  the  two  behaviors  to 
two  different  dimensions  and  levels  of  personality.  The  logical  classi- 
fication which  results  has  further  implications  in  the  broader  field  of 
artistic  interpretation.  Whenever  psychologists  venture  to  apply  their 
theories  to  the  aesthetic  and  creative  realm  they  should,  and  usually 
do,  make  it  clear  that  their  analyses  refer  not  to  the  form  but  to  the 
content  of  the  artistic  production.  Psychologists  have  made  many 
brilliant  expositions  of  the  thematic  meaning  of  creative  expression, 
but  they  have  properly  said  little  as  to  the  artistic  or  formal  merit. 
They  attempt  to  understand  ivhat  the  artist  is  communicating  and  not 
hoiv  skillfully  he  is  expressing  it. 

The  logic  of  levels  makes  this  distinction  quite  clear.  Our  judg- 
ments of  the  form,  the  style,  the  manner  of  behavior  is  a  Level  I  op- 
eration. We  are  rating  noninterpersonal  behavior.  Our  judgments  as 
to  the  meaning  and  thematic  message  being  communicated  is  a  Level 
III  operation.  We  are  rating  symbolic  expression. 

The  Function  of  Symbols 

In  the  last  few  pages  we  have  been  circling  around  the  general  ques- 
tion of  the  meaning  of  symbohc  activity.  Symbols  are  not  necessarily 
the  reverse  of  the  coin  of  consciousness  as  the  theory  of  opposites 
would  lead  us  to  believe.  Nor  are  they  always  the  behaviors  tapped 
by  the  so-called  projective  tests.  We  shall  now  consider  some  answers 
to  the  questions:  "What  is  the  function,  meaning,  and  purpose  of 
symbols,  and  what  is  their  practical  clinical  use?" 

Symbols  Are  a  Private  "Preconscious"  Language.  First,  it  can 
be  said  that  imaginative  expressions  are  a  form  of  communication,  an 
indirect  form.  The  individual  does  not  tell  us  directly  about  himself; 
he  describes  a  fantasy  or  unreal  set  of  events.  Symbols  are  the  vocab- 
ulary of  a  private  language.  When  the  subject  talks  directly  about 
himself  (in  Level  II)  he  is  describing  himself  to  another  person.  He 
is  telling  the  psychiatrist,  the  tester,  or  the  other  patients  in  his  group 
about  his  perceptions  of  self  and  world.  When  he  talks  in  the  sym- 
bolic language  of  dream  or  fantasy  he  is  not  telling  others  about  him- 
self or  his  real  world.  He  may  be,  in  a  sense,  talking  to  himself.  Sym- 
bolic language  is  inexplicable  and  mysterious  if  we  try  to  interpret  it 
as  though  the  person  were  talking  directly  and  openly  about  his  con- 


1 62  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

scious  perceptions.  When  interpreted  as  private  language,  we  see  that 
it  can  be  understood  only  in  the  context  of  its  personal  meaning  to  the 
subject. 

It  is  very  well  known  that  all  individuals  have  a  set  of  private  per- 
ceptions, private  opinions,  and  private  reactions  which  often  contrast 
with  the  statements  of  conscious  report.  This  has  classically  been  the 
despair  of  philosophers  who  have  had  to  concede  that  we  can  never 
know  exactly  what  goes  on  in  the  mind  of  another  human  being.  It  has 
made  the  topic  of  "consciousness"  the  source  of  unending  speculative 
frustration.  The  first  step  in  approaching  this  riddle  is  to  accept  the 
inevitable  limitations  and  indeterminacy  involved.  The  second  step  is 
to  develop  the  best  means  for  getting  as  close  as  possible  to  the  "pre- 
conscious"  or  private  world  of  our  fellow  men.  At  the  present  time, 
this  can  be  best  accomplished  through  the  language  of  symbolism.  ' 

This  is  by  no  means  a  simple  or  unambiguous  procedure.  In  many 
cases  the  subject  is  made  quite  anxious  if  he  attempts  to  translate  his 
own  private  expressions  into  the  language  of  direct  conscious  descrip- 
tion. In  many  cases  he  is  made  even  more  anxious  at  the  prospect  of 
others  approaching  his  idiom.  The  expression  and  interpretation  of 
symbols  is  loaded  with  complicating  qualifications.  Their  meaning  al- 
ways depends  on  the  dynamics  of  the  total  personality  and  of  the 
context  in  which  the  symbols  are  expressed.  Thus  the  level  of  per- 
sonality which  includes  imaginative  indirect  communications  offers, 
on  the  one  hand,  the  most  promising  avenue  to  the  private  world  of 
the  subject,  and  involves,  on  the  other  hand,  the  most  ambiguity  and 
interpretive  uncertainty. 

Symbols  Reduce  Anxiety.  Next  we  must  consider  the  function 
of  symbolic  behavior.  Why  do  human  beings  develop  private  lan- 
guages? The  first  answer  to  this  question  seems  to  follow  quite  logi- 
cally; they  develop  indirect  behaviors  to  avoid  the  anxiety  of  the  direct. 
They  express  certain  themes  privately  to  avoid  the  anxiety  that  pub- 
lic expression  would  entail. 

We  have  seen  in  earlier  chapters  that  all  individuals  develop  auto- 
matic interpersonal  response  preferences.  They  use  some  favored  re- 
actions and  avoid  others  which  would  involve  greater  anxiety.  By 
means  of  the  language  of  symbolism  it  is  possible  to  express  inter- 
personal themes  that  are  inhibited  from  direct  expression.  We  are  re- 
turning here  to  the  old  principle  of  expression  of  the  opposite  which, 
we  have  learned  from  the  data,  works  only  part  of  the  time.  We  must 
complete  the  explanation  by  adding  the  other  end  of  the  continuum: 
by  means  of  the  language  of  symbolism  it  is  also  possible  to  repeat  and 
thus  strengthen  the  same  themes  that  are  manifested  in  direct  expres- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  163 

sion  and  to  avoid  further  the  themes  that  are  inhibited  from  direct  ex- 
pression. The  purpose  of  symbolic  behavior  is  to  reduce  anxiety. 
For  some  individuals  this  is  accomplished  by  employing  fantasy  as  a 
safety  valve,  an  opportunity  to  "blow  off"  the  interpersonal  steam  that 
has  built  up  through  inhibitions  and  repressions.  For  others,  even  in- 
direct, imaginative  expression  of  the  inhibited  themes  is  anxiety-laden. 
SymboHc  behavior  in  these  cases  becomes  a  way  of  strengthening  the 
avoidance  maneuvers. 

Symbolic  Mode  Indicates  the  Source  and  Amount  of  Anxiety. 
Thus  we  see  that  there  is  no  simple,  one-way  explanation  for  the  mean- 
ing of  symbolic  language  that  works  for  all  cases.  We  lose  the  com- 
forting simplicity  of  a  generalized  rule.  But  we  gain,  instead,  a  new 
illuminating  hypothesis:  symbolic  language  can  serve  as  an  index  as  to 
the  amount  and  source  of  the  subject's  anxiety.  The  patient  who 
rigidly  limits  his  direct  interpersonal  activity  at  Level  I  to  a  few  nar- 
rowed responses  and  avoids  all  others  can  go  on  to  develop  any  num- 
ber of  symbolic  resolutions.  If  the  rigid  limitation  continues  in  his 
imaginative  productions  then  we  can  assume  that  the  anxiety  which 
cripples  and  inhibits  the  absent  interpersonal  themes  is  so  intense  that 
he  cannot  express  them  even  indirectly  in  the  private  language  of 
symbols.'^  If,  on  the  contrary,  the  themes  which  are  avoided  in  con- 
scious report  or  pubhc  communication  appear  at  the  level  of  private 
conversation,  we  can  make  the  hypothesis  that  the  anxiety  is  less  crip- 
pling and  that  increased  flexibility,  mobility,  and  potential  for  change 
exist.  And  we  have,  further,  a  clue  as  to  the  direction  of  the  antici- 
pated change,  as  indicated  by  the  new  themes  that  appear  in  the  priv- 
ate language  of  symbols.  These  hypotheses  bear  up  under  the  ob- 
jective test  (see  Appendix  3). 

Symbols  Can  Express  Underlying  Feelings  of  Uniqueness  and 
Self-Consolation.  We  interpret  Level  III  productions  in  the  light  of 
the  total  personality.  It  is  obvious  that  symbols  can  present  the  same 
thematic  picture  as  the  other  levels,  they  can  be  "more  so,"  or  they  can 
be  different.  If  the  latter  is  true,  then  the  individual  has  expressed  in 
fantasy  the  themes  he  has  inhibited  in  public  communications.  He  may 
say  publicly,  "I  am  meek  and  weak  and  suffering."  Now  he  may  add 
the  private  comment,  "but  I  am  also  concerned  with  the  theme  of  re- 
taliation, or  power,  or  prestige."  To  present  this  illustration  in  other 
words,  the  individual  is  saying:  "I  tell  you  openly  that  I  am  submissive, 

^  The  cultural  situation  in  which  the  symbols  are  expressed  is  a  crucial  factor. 
Powelson  and  Bendix  (9)  have  described  the  effect  that  a  punitive,  custodial  environ- 
ment can  have  on  patients'  behavior.  The  cultural  context  must  be  added  as  a  qualify- 
ing variable  to  all  the  generalizations  made  in  this  chapter. 


164  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

but  I  wish,  or  hope,  or  symbolically  perceive  myself  to  be  strong  and 
powerful."  Self-esteem  is  increased  and  anxiety  diminished  by  the 
secret  fantasy  of  fearful  power. 

This  kind  of  self-punitive  masochist  in  fantasy  asserts  himself  and 
retaliates  against  his  tormenters.  This  is  a  familiar  tune.  It  illustrates 
the  notion  of  reversal.  We  have  designated  this  as  an  interlevel  con- 
flict— masochism  at  Level  II  opposed  to  sadistic  assertion  at  Level  III. 
But  how  about  the  masochist  whose  fantasy  productions  are  saturated 
with  even  more  self-defeat?  How  is  this  explained  in  terms  of  warded 
off  anxiety  and  the  theory  of  stabilization?  These  persons — and  there 
are  many  of  them — are  convinced  at  all  levels  of  expression  that  suf- 
fering and  self-abasement  is  the  safest,  least  threatening  method  of  ad- 
justment. They  are,  we  assume,  less  anxious  when  they  express  maso- 
chistic themes.  They  appear  to  get  some  consolation,  excuse,  and 
poignant  merit  from  unhappy  fantasies. 

These  patients  seem  to  be  saying  something  like  this:  "I  am  overtly 
meek,  weak  and  suffering  .  .  .  and  covertly  I  do  not  perceive  my- 
self as  anything  different."  Often  the  pessimistic  corollary  is:  "I  dare 
not  change  or  I  do  not  wish  to  change."  Patients  may  enhance  their 
esteem  and  feelings  of  uniqueness  by  means  of  their  private  symbols: 
"No  one  is  as  uniquely  unloved,  helpless,  and  martyred  as  I  am." 

The  conceptualization  of  masochistic  behavior  is  traditionally  the 
proving  ground  on  which  personality  theories  meet  their  most  taxing 
tests.  It  is  the  point  where  the  logical  assumptions  based  on  survival 
value  begin  to  buckle  and  where  new  concepts,  such  as  death  instincts, 
are  classically  dragged  into  action.  We  have  attempted  in  the  last  few 
pages  to  employ  a  motivating  principle — avoidance  of  anxiety  and 
preservation  of  self-esteem — to  explain  symbolic  activity  in  the  same 
terms  as  public  behavior. 

Time-Binding  Nature  of  Symbols.  With  these  remarks  as  pre- 
liminary it  is  now  possible  to  present  the  essential  point  of  this  chapter. 
Symbolic,  indirect  or  ''''pre conscious"  activities  are  necessary  for  the 
human  being  because  he  is  a  time-binding  individual. 

Unlike  most  other  mammals,  the  human  being  continually  faces  and 
deals  with  conflictful  situations  in  which  anxiety  threatens  in  at  least 
two  directions.  The  interpersonal  world  he  has  created  pushes  him 
toward  one  set  (and  often  an  imbalanced  set)  of  anxiety  reducing  be- 
haviors. The  pressures  toward  flexibility,  both  cultural  and  personal, 
may  push  him  toward  another  source  of  self-esteem.  The  individual's 
overt  behavior  does  not  express  the  impulse  or  desire  which  he  feels. 

This  point  has  been  well  made  by  Murray  and  Kluckhohn  (5  p.  18). 
They  point  out  that:  ".  .  .  the  personality  is  almost  continuously  in- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  165 

volved  in  deciding  between  alternative  or  conflicting  tendencies  or 
elements."  Personalities  deal  with  these  conflicts  by  constructing 
"schedules  which  permit  the  execution  of  as  many  connotations  as  pos- 
sible, one  after  the  other."  They  go  on  to  say,  "Most  men  are  forced 
by  circumstances  to  make  decisions  which  commit  them  to  schedules 
arranged  by  others  (e.g.,  the  daily  routine  of  a  job);  and  so  a  large 
portion  of  the  temporal  order  of  their  days  is  not  of  their  own  shap- 
ing. Also,  every  culture  prescribes  schedules,  general  and  special, 
which  define  the  proper  time,  place  or  order  of  certain  actions,  and, 
therefore,  schedule-making  is  a  sphere  in  which  the  individual  is  likely 
to  come  into  conflict  with  his  society." 

Postponement  of  impulse  is  thus  an  inevitable  characteristic  of  hu- 
man behavior.  The  individual  is  continually  inhibiting  some  actions 
in  favor  of  others,  generally  moving  in  the  direction  of  the  lesser  anx- 
iety. 

This  postponement  phenomenon  is  called  the  time-binding  aspect 
of  human  behavior.  The  function  of  "preconscious"  or  "unconscious" 
activities  might  thus  be  explained  as  time-binding.  The  basic  discovery 
of  Freud  that  unexpressed  impulses  do  not  disappear  but  remain  as 
active,  although  indirect,  elements  in  the  personality  can  be  considered 
as  a  temporal  rather  than  a  structural  phenomenon.  The  unexpressed 
motives  relate  to  the  past  and  the  future. 

From  the  functional  viewpoint,  the  essence  of  private  or  "precon- 
scious" factors  is  that  they  are  potentials  for  later  overt  or  at  least 
conscious  expression.  If  they  did  not  have  this  potentiality  then  they 
have  little  meaning.  This  time-binding  theory  of  the  "preconscious" 
is  important  and  useful  in  the  interpretation  of  symbolic  behavior. 

Whenever  we  obtain  a  symbolic,  "preconscious"  theme  from  a  sub- 
ject, it  suggests  that  this  theme  is  a  potential  for  future  action.  The 
time-binding  theory  of  the  "preconscious"  places  the  symbol  pro- 
duced in  the  present  on  a  temporal  dimension  pointing  (we  assume) 
to  earlier  frustration  and  functionally  more  important  to  a  later  ex- 
pression of  the  theme. 

Symbols  Predict  Future  Behavior.  The  functional  value  of  sym- 
bolic behavior  to  the  clinician  can  now  be  stated.  The  data  of  Level 
III  are  predictions  of  the  future.  We  have  proposed  the  hypothesis 
that  the  patient's  symbols  tell  us,  in  the  case  of  conflict  between  con- 
scious and  "preconscious"  themes:  "Here  is  another  side  of  my  inter- 
personal picture."  In  the  case  where  the  fantasy  themes  are  not  dis- 
crepant from  overt  behavior  he  tells  us:  "My  rigid  pattern  does  not 
change  even  in  symbols."  The  "preconscious"  themes  tell  us  how 
likely  the  person  is  to  change  his  behavior  and  in  what  direction  he  is 


,56  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

likely  to  change.  The  usefulness  of  this  information  in  clinical  prac- 
tice is  obvious. 

If  this  theory  is  correct,  then  symbols  should  indicate  the  amount  of 
anxiety  that  operates  in  any  given  personality  structure,  and  they 
should  give  us  an  estimation  of  the  amount  of  change  to  be  expected 
and  the  type  of  change  to  be  expected. 

If  this  theory  is  correct,  a  prognostic  instrument  of  considerable 
importance  becomes  available  to  clinicians.  And  conversely,  if  the 
predictions  do  hold  up  when  applied  to  clinical  practice,  a  major  theo- 
retical step  will  have  been  taken  in  explaining  that  area  of  personality 
which  has  always  been  so  resistant  to  explanation.  In  helping  the 
clinician  predict,  we  shall  have  validated  the  theory  of  symbols. 

In  order  to  test  this  hypothesis,  several  methodological  problems 
had  to  be  met.  It  was  necessary  to  convert  the  loose,  diverse  language 
of  symbols  into  scientific  categories.  The  interpersonal  variables  thus 
defined  must  be  capable  of  reliable  measurement.  They  must  be  di- 
rectly and  systematically  related  to  the  other  levels  of  personality  so 
that  interlevel  conflicts  and  discrepancies  can  be  measured. 

The  data  for  Level  III,  it  will  be  remembered,  are  defined  auto- 
matically by  the  source  from  which  they  come.  A  dream,  a  fantasy,  or 
any  projective  and  imaginative  expression  reported  by  a  subject  is 
assigned  to  the  symbolic  mode.  The  "preconscious"  level,  like  the 
more  overt  levels,  is  divided  into  two  areas:  self  and  other,  or  symbolic 
hero  and  symbolic  nvorld.  This  division  produces  two  distinctly  dif- 
ferent types  of  Level  III  material  which  have  unique  applications  and 
lawful  relationships  to  other  levels  of  personality. 

Six  Methods  for  Measuring  Level  III  Behavior 

There  are  six  methods  which  have  been  employed  by  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  project  for  measuring  Level  III  behavior.  Whenever  the 
discussion  centers  on  the  generic  level  of  "preconscious"  expression  we 
used  the  code  Level  III.  Whenever  we  refer  to  specific  measurements 
of  "preconscious"  behavior  it  is  necessary  to  indicate  the  specific 
source  of  the  data  by  adding  the  appropriate  code  letter.  This  is  ac- 
complished as  follows: 

When  trained  personnel  rate  the  interpersonal  content  of  responses 
from  the  Iflund  projective  test  (4)  the  scores  are  labeled  Level  lU-i. 
Scores  for  the  Blacky  projective  test  (1)  are  coded  Level  lll-B. 

When  the  interpersonal  themes  for  dreams  are  rated  by  trained  per- 
sonnel the  scores  are  indicated  as  Level  lll-D.  Themes  from  waking 
fantasies  are  coded  Level  lll-F. 

MMPI  indices  which  are  being  developed  to  predict  preconscious 
behavior  are  coded  Level  lll-M. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  167 

When  trained  personnel  rate  the  interpersonal  themes  from  the 
Thematic  Aperception  Test  (TAT)  the  scores  are  labeled  Level  lll-T. 
The  research  studies  reported  in  this  book  employ  the  TAT  as  the 
standard  instrument  for  Level  III. 

When  trained  personnel  rate  the  interpersonal  themes  from  the 
Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test  the  scores  are  coded  Level  III-IFT.^ 

The  hypothesis  that  "preconscious"  symbols  predict  future  be- 
havior to  be  expected  at  overt  or  conscious  levels  has  been  tested  by 
means  of  several  research  studies.  The  design  and  detailed  results  are 
presented  in  Appendix  3.  These  findings  do  confirm  this  hypothesis. 
"Preconscious"  behavior  does  predict  the  kind  and  the  amount  of 
change  to  be  expected  in  future  overt  behavior.  If  the  TAT,  for  ex- 
ample, is  more  hostile  than  the  self-diagnosis  at  the  first  testing,  then 
the  self-diagnosis  can  be  expected  to  change  over  time  in  the  direction 
of  greater  hostility. 

The  methodology  for  measuring  symbolic  behavior  which  is  used 
in  clinical  diagnosis  and  in  these  validating  research  studies  will  now  be 
presented. 

The  Measurement  of  Interpersonal  Symbols 

The  raw  data  of  Level  III  comprise  the  verbal  language  of  the 
dream  texts,  fantasy  stories,  projective  test  protocols.  The  task  here 
is  the  same  as  at  any  other  level  of  personality — to  convert  the  raw 
protocol  language  into  objective  categories.  One  of  the  basic  prin- 
ciples of  the  interpersonal  system  of  personality  is  that  the  same  matrix 
of  variables  should  be  used  at  every  level  of  behavior.  This  facilitates 
direct  comparison  between  levels. 

This  principle  settles  in  advance  the  issue  of  what  variables  are  to  be 
used  in  measuring  symbolic  behavior.  The  sixteen-variable  circular 
continuum  by  which  we  rate  public  communications  and  conscious 
descriptions  is  also  employed  to  analyze  the  imaginative  data. 

Classification  of  Fantasy  Materials  into  ''Hero''  and  ''Others'' 

We  rate  the  heroes  and  protagonists  of  the  fantasy  world  just  the 
way  we  rate  the  activity  of  the  subject's  real  self  and  real  ivorld  at 
Levels  II  and  III.  If  the  dream  hero  is  fearful,  he  is  scored  H;  if  he  is 
murderously  enraged,  we  score  E,  etc.  The  same  procedure  of  differ- 
entiating between  the  self  and  the  other  is  preserved.  The  hero  of  the 

^  The  Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test  is  a  projective  instrument  developed  to  tap  the 
subject's  fantasy  descriptions  of  the  permutations  and  combinations  of  the  most  stand- 
ard and  crucial  interpersonal  relationships.  It  is  tailored  to  the  interpersonal  system 
just  as  the  Interpersonal  Check  List  is  designed  to  tap  the  sixteen  variable  continuum. 
The  Interpersonal  Fantasy  Test  is  published  by  the  Psychological  Consultation  Service, 
Berkeley,  California. 


,58  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

dream  becomes  the  symbolic  self.  The  people  he  interacts  with  de- 
note the  interpersonal  other.  The  identities  of  these  figures  of  the 
interpersonal  world  are  specified  much  as  they  are  at  Levels  I  and  II. 
Formal  rules  and  conventions  for  determining  which  character  in  a 
dream  or  fantasy  story  is  the  hero  and  which  are  the  "others"  have 
been  developed.    (See  Appendix  3.) 

This  division  into  "preconscious"  hero  and  "preconscious"  other 
is  an  important  one.  It  defines  two  separate  sublevels  of  the  "pre- 
conscious." Studies  taken  from  several  widely  differing  samples  con- 
sistently show  that  the  "preconscious"  other  scores  are  significantly 
different  from  the  "preconscious"  self  or  hero  scores. 

The  establishment  of  an  operationally  defined  hierarchy  of  "pre- 
conscious" layers  has  functional  value.  It  helps  us  understand  the 
amount  of  anxiety  connected  to  any  interpersonal  behavior.  If  an 
emotion — let  us  say,  rebelliousness — appears  at  Level  II,  we  can  as- 
sume that  the  subject  is  not  made  so  anxious  by  the  emotion  that  he 
must  avoid  it  consciously.  He  can  tolerate  this  interpersonal  behavior 
in  conscious  report.  If  he  completely  avoids  or  denies  it  at  Levels 
I  and  II,  one  assumes  its  direct  expression  makes  him  anxious. 

If,  in  this  latter  case,  the  rebelliousness  appears  at  Level  III  hero, 
this  indicates  that  he  can  tolerate  the  emotion  at  the  "top  layer  of 
fantasy."  This  indicates  that  the  anxiety  connected  with  unconven- 
tional behavior  is  not  too  massive  or  crippling. 

Let  us  consider  the  case  where  rebelhousness  is  completely  avoided 
at  Levels  I,  II,  and  Level  III  hero.  This  suggests  that  the  three  most 
overt  levels  are  organized  against  the  expression  of  the  emotion. 
Considerable  anxiety  must  be  connected  with  its  expression. 

At  this  point  the  distinction  between  Level  III  hero  and  other  be- 
comes functionally  useful.  Since  there  is  evidence  that  Level  III  other 
is  deeper  than  Level  III  hero,  we  look  to  the  themes  attributed  to  the 
fantasy  world.  If  they,  too,  are  marked  by  an  avoidance  of  rebellious- 
ness, we  have  an  added  cue  as  to  the  amount  of  anxiety  attached.  If 
the  themes  warded  off  at  the  three  more  overt  layers  finally  appear  in 
the  fantasy  world,  then  they  become  potentially  available  for  future 
integration  into  awareness. 

This  brings  us,  of  course,  to  a  classic  item  of  clinical  folklore — it  is 
easier  for  the  patient  to  master  and  integrate  feelings  which  are  pro- 
jected onto  others  than  if  they  do  not  appear  at  all.  Pulling  back 
projections  and  accepting  them  into  the  self-structure  is  a  basic  thera- 
peutic procedure.  The  differentiation  of  layers  of  "preconscious" 
behavior  is  a  useful  device  with  considerable  cHnical  application. 
Preconscious  themes  attributed  to  fantasy  others  are  thus  considered 
part  of  the  subject's  personality,  usually  related  meaningfully  to  the 
conscious  or  preconscious  self. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  i6g 

Classification  of  Fantasy  Images 

There  is  a  further  differentiation  of  "preconscious"  behavior  which 
has  certain  theoretical  and  chnical  implications.  The  fantasy  stories 
are  first  divided  into  self  or  hero  and  the  themes  attributed  to  each  are 
scored  according  to  interpersonal  variables  (see  next  section).  After 
the  main  hero-world  themes  are  scored,  then  each  character  is  classi- 
fied according  to  his  familial  status.  The  categories  employed  are 
maternal  figures,  paternal  figures,  cross-sex  figures,  and  same-sex  fig- 
ures. It  is  then  possible  to  add  up  the  scores  which  summarize  the  re- 
lationship between: 

Male  child  vs.  Maternal  figure 

Male  child  vs.  Paternal  figure 

Female  child  vs.  Maternal  figure 

Female  child  vs.  Paternal  figure 

Fantasy  figures  of  subject's  sex  vs.  Cross-sex  figure 

Fantasy  figures  of  subject's  sex  vs.  Same-sex  figure 

These  procedures  give  a  summary  of  the  interpersonal  behavior  as- 
signed to  these  important  role  relationships  at  the  level  of  fantasy.  In 
a  later  chapter  we  shall  consider  indices  which  systematically  link  these 
interlevel  processes,  e.g.,  the  similarity  or  difference  between  the  con- 
scious and  "preconscious"  views  of  maternal  figures.  The  classifica- 
tion of  "preconscious"  personages  make  possible  the  operational  defi- 
nition of  such  processes  as  displacement,  "preconscious"  identification, 
and  the  like. 

Symbols  Involve  Greater  Violence  and  Intensity 

There  is  a  difference  between  the  rating  of  Level  III  themes  and 
overt  behavior.  The  actions  and  traits  expressed  in  the  symbolic  mode 
are  usually  much  richer  than  those  of  the  other  levels.  More  intensity 
of  feeling  and  violence  of  action  occurs.  Patients  rarely  describe  mur- 
ders or  world-shaking  power  motives  in  their  conscious  reports  about 
their  real  lives.  In  their  Level  I  observed  interactions  in  group  therapy, 
the  patients  may  insult  or  help  each  other — but  blood  never  flows. 
The  acts  of  generosity  are  limited  and  generally  expressed  verbally. 

This  is,  of  course,  not  so  in  fantasy.  Intense  affect,  crime,  rape,  sui- 
cidal grief,  physical  and  material  generosity,  bodily  exchanges  of  love 
and  hate  often  occur.  The  same  interpersonal  motives  appear  to 
underlie  the  interactions  of  both  levels.  The  sixteen-variable  con- 
tinuum seems  adequate  to  categorize  the  behavior.  It  is  the  manner  in 
which  the  interpersonal  purpose  is  expressed  that  is  different. 


lyo  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

For  this  reason  additions  to  the  rating  system  have  been  made  in 
order  to  handle  symbolic  data.  The  list  of  specific  ways  in  which  the 
same  interpersonal  purposes  can  be  manifested  must  be  expanded.  In 
a  therapy  group  or  discussion  group  the  motive  D  can  be  expressed 
through  sarcasm,  disapproval,  punitive  comments,  derision,  verbal 
threats,  etc.  In  fantasy  the  motive  D  can  be  expressed  in  these  same 
terms,  but  in  addition  in  a  more  intense  manner.  These  generally  in- 
volve physical  or  material  modes,  brutal  punishments,  actual  destruc- 
tion, incarceration,  execution,  etc. 

The  illustrative  key  employed  in  analyzing  symbolic  activity, 
therefore,  includes  all  the  actions  measured  at  Levels  II  and  I — but  it 
also  includes  the  more  intense  and  violent  activities  of  the  fantasy 
world. 

The  Variables  of  Level  III 

Table  4  presents  a  hst  of  sample  behaviors  as  rated  at  Level  III.  It 
must  be  kept  in  mind  the  list  of  verbs  used  at  Level  I  and  the  list  of 
attributes  used  at  Level  II  also  apply  in  the  symbolic  mode.  We  have 
not  duplicated  these  lists  of  behaviors  here,  but  have  listed  just  the 
interpersonal  events  which  are  unique  to  Level  III. 

TABLE  4 

Illustrative  Classification  of  Interpersonal 
Behavior  at  the  Symbolic  or  Projective  Level 

A.  The  code  A  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Power:  Leadership,  Command,  Direction, 
Authority. 

B.  The  code  B  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Narcissism:  Independence,  Self-Expression, 
Superior,  Power  Struggle. 

C.  The  code  C  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Exploitation:  Seduction,  Rape,  Rejecting, 
Depriving,  Selfishness,  Keeping  Away  From,  Keeping  Children  to  Self. 

D.  The  code  D  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Punitive  Hostility:  Punishment,  Coercion, 
Brutality,  Quarreling,  Threat. 

E.  The  code  E  is  assigned  to  themes  of  All  Forms  of  Pure  Hostility:  Disaffiliation, 
Murder,  Anger,  Fighting. 

F.  The  code  F  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Unconventional  Activity:  Passive  Resistance, 
Rebellion,  Generic  Crime  versus  Authority,  Pure  Jealousy,  Drunkenness,  Stealing 
Covertly,  Offended,  Bitterness. 

G.  The  code  G  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Deprivation:  Distrust,  Disappointment,  Re- 
jectedness.  Suspicion,  Bad  Things  Are  Done  to  One. 

H.  The  code  H  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Masochism:  Grief,  Suicide,  Withdrawal, 
Guilt,  Provoking  Punishment,  Self -Punishment,  Fear,  Anxiety,  Insanity  (Unspeci- 
fied), Loneliness,  Running  Away. 

I.  The  code  /  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Weakness:  Obedience,  Submission,  Uncon- 
sciousness, Indecision,  Ambivalence,  Immobilization,  Illness,  Passivity. 

J.  The  code  /  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Conformity:  Accepting  Advice,  Provoking 
Advice,  Being  Student,  Docility,  Followership,  Positive  Passivity. 

K.  The  code  K  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Trust:  Cling,  Good  Things  Come  to  One, 
Good  Luck,  Being  Taken  Care  of.  Dependence,  Gratitude. 

L.    The  code  L  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Collaboration  and  Agreeability:  Congeniality, 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


71 


Cooperation,  "Generic  Happy  Ending"  Caused  by  People  Working  Things  Out, 
Adjustment  in  General. 

M.  The  code  M  is  assigned  to  themes  of  All  Forms  of  Pure  Love:  Affiliation,  Mar- 
riage, Friendship. 

N.  The  code  N  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Tenderness:  Support,  Kindness,  Encourage- 
ment, Solace,  Pity. 

O.  The  code  O  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Generosity:  Help,  Curing  Someone,  Taking 
Care  of  Someone,  Giving. 

P.  The  code  P  is  assigned  to  themes  of  Success:  Heroism,  Popularity,  Acclaim, 
Achievement,  Wisdom,  Teaching,  Explaining. 

The  cautionary  statements  made  when  we  listed  sample  themes  for 
Levels  I  and  II  must  be  repeated  again.  This  table  of  themes  is  illus- 
trative. Actually  the  list  of  potential  symbolic  themes  is  exhausted 
only  by  the  seemingly  infinite  variety  of  man's  autism  and  creativity. 
We  have  included  here  only  the  most  common.  (A  glance  at  Table  4 
makes  apparent  the  great  variety  of  behavior  tapped  in  the  symbolic 
mode.  Any  aspect  of  human  experience  can  appear  at  Level  III:  sexual 
events,  relations  to  authority,  law,  nature,  occupational  and  political 
adjustments,  the  vicissitudes  of  childhood,  mating,  marriage,  parent- 
hood, of  growing  up  and  growing  old.)  The  illustrative  words  in 
Table  4  tap  only  a  small  fraction  of  the  potential.  We  have  concen- 
trated, therefore,  on  the  events  most  commonly  obtained  in  response 
to  a  standard  projective  test — the  Thematic  Apperception  Test. 

In  actual  practice  the  rating  is  assigned  not  in  a  routine,  check-off 
fashion,  but  by  a  judgmental  application  of  the  circular  concept. 
That  is,  the  list  of  themes  presented  in  this  illustrative  figure  is  not  ap- 
plied automatically.  Murder  can  be  scored  as  F,  E,  or  D  depending  on 
the  power  element  involved  in  the  hostility.  Success  is  B  if  it  involves 
superiority  or  proving  someone  else  inferior;  it  can  be  F  if  it  involves 
the  notion  of  respect  or  admiration  from  others. 

Here  are  examples  of  the  interpersonal  system  applied  to  three 
varieties  of  Level  III  data:  a  dream  of  Sigmund  Freud  (Level  III-D), 
a  fantasy  (Level  III-F),  and  a  projective  test  story  (Level  III-T), 

The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Symbols 
from  a  Dream  of  Sigmund  Freud 

Freud  in  The  Interpretation  of  Dreams  (2,  pp.  195-96)  presents 
a  protocol  which  can  be  used  to  illustrate  the  scoring  of  interpersonal 
symbols.  This  dream  concerns  "preconscious"  transference-counter- 
transference  feelings  on  the  part  of  Freud  and  provides  an  example  of 
how  Level  III  self  and  other  scores  are  employed  to  yield  different 
measurements. 

Freud  prefaces  this  dream  with  the  explanation  that  Irma  was  a 
patient  whose  analysis  ended  only  "in  partial  success."  "I  expected  her 


172 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


to  accept  a  solution  which  did  not  seem  acceptable  to  her."  Later  a 
friend  reported  to  Freud  that  Irma  "was  not  quite  well."  Freud  re- 
ports he  was  annoyed  by  the  possible  reproach  in  the  friend's  voice 
"that  same  evening  I  wrote  the  clinical  history  of  Irma's  case,  in  order 
to  give  it,  as  though  to  justify  myself.  .  .  ." 

These  Level  II  conscious  descriptions  of  Freud  would  be  scored 
as  follows: 


Other 


Protocol  Description  Self 

"I  expected  her  to  accept  a  solution  which        A  or  B 

did  not  seem  acceptable  to  her."  F 

A  friend  reproaches  Freud.  D 

This  "annoyed  me."  E 

The  patient's  relatives  did  not  approve  of 
the  treatment.  D 

Freud  tries  to  justify  himself.  B 

The  relationship  between  Freud  and  the  patient  is  summarized  as: 
Freud  =  A  or  B  < — >  Patient  =  F 

The  relationship  between  Freud  and  the  friend  and  relatives  of  the 
patient  is: 

Freud  =  E  and  B  < — >  Friend  =  D 

Freud's  conscious  description  of  self  in  this  episode  locates  in  the 
upper  left-hand  quadrant  of  the  interpersonal  circle.  He  is  strong, 
right,  and  righteously  angry. 

The  night  following  these  events  Freud  had  a  dream.  We  shall 
consider  the  first  secrion  of  the  dream  which  relates  to  the  relationship 
to  the  patient. 


Dream  of  July  23-24,  1895 


Freud 


Patient 


A  great  hall— a  number  of  guests,  whom  we  are  re- 
ceiving—among them  Irma,  whom  I  immediately  take    A— directs 
aside,  as  though  to  answer  her  letter,  and  to  reproach    D— reproaches 
her  for  not  yet  accepting  the  "solution."   I  say  to  her: 
"If  you  still  have  pains,  it  is  really  only  your  own 
fault."— She  answers:  "If  you  only  knew  what  pains  I 
have  now  in  the  throat,  stomach,  and  abdomen— I  am 
choked  by  them."  I  am  stanled,  and  look  at  her.  She 
looks  pale  and  puffy.  I  think  that  after  all  I  must  be 
overlooking  some  organic  affection.  I  take  her  to  the    A— directs 
window  and  look  into  her  throat.    She  offers  some 
resistance  to  this,  like  a  woman  who  has  a  set  of  false    D—is  critical 
teeth.   I  think,  surely,  she  doesn't  need  them. 

The  scoring  of  this  dream  indicates  that  Freud's  "preconscious' 
view  of  his  relationship  with  this  patient  is  as  follows: 


F— complains 
I— acts  weak 


F-rebels 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


173 


Freud's  "preconscious"  self      =  Strong,  right,  punitive 
Freud's  "preconscious"  other  =  Weak  and  rebelUous 

At  this  point  we  have  data  from  three  layers  indicating  Freud's 
reaction  to  this  interpersonal  situation.  At  Level  II  Freud  is  strong 
and  self-confident.  At  the  next  deeper  level  of  personality,  i.e..  Level 
III-D  hero,  he  is  strong  and  righteously  angry.  At  Level  III-D  other, 
he  attributes  to  others  rebellious  and  weak  themes.  This  last  statement 
requires  comment.  We  no  longer  consider  Level  III  other  as  being  a 
characteristic  of  the  "other  one,"  but  as  a  deeper  estimate  of  the  sub- 
ject's own  feelings  which  have  been  projected  onto  "preconscious" 
images.  The  Level  III  other  can  be  seen  as  an  underlying  identifica- 
tion with  weakness. 

A  three-layer  summary  of  Freud's  reactions  to  a  rather  difficult  in- 
terpersonal situation  thus  results.  We  see  that  at  two  top  levels  (Level 
I  is,  of  course,  not  available)  Freud  was  maintaining  a  position  of 
strength,  self-confidence,  and  sternness.  At  the  deeper  layer  (Level 
Ill-other)  there  is  evidence  suggesting  that  Freud  felt  resentful  and 
threatened  by  this  experience.  His  rebeUiousness  and  feelings  of  weak- 
ness, we  assume,  were  accompanied  by  some  anxiety  because  they  ap- 
pear at  Level  III-D  other,  projected  onto  the  image  of  the  patient. 
This  anxiety  was  not  crippling  as  indicated  by  Freud's  frank  descrip- 
tion of  his  feelings. 

A  classic  example  of  fantasy  behavior  is  found  in  the  short  story 
"The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty"  by  James  Thurber  (10).  A  sum- 
mary of  his  overt  behavior  is  contained  in  the  following  episode: 

Level  I-R  Behavior  of  Walter  Mitty  as  Scored  by  the  Interpersonal  System 


Self 


F— passively  resists 


l-obeys 

F— passively  resists 


Walter  Mitty  stopped  the  car  in  front  of  the 
building  where  his  wife  went  to  have  her  hair 
done.  "Remember  to  get  those  overshoes  while 
I'm  having  my  hair  done,"  she  said.  "I  don't 
need  overshoes,"  said  Mitty.  She  put  her  mirror 
back  into  her  bag.  "We've  been  all  through 
that,"  she  said,  getting  out  of  the  car.  "You're 
not  a  young  man  any  longer."  He  raced  the 
engine  a  little.  "Why  don't  you  wear  your 
gloves?  Have  you  lost  your  gloves?"  Walter 
Mitty  reached  into  a  pocket  and  brought  out 
the  gloves.  He  put  them  on,  but  after  she  had 
turned  and  gone  into  the  building  and  he  had 
driven  on  to  a  red  light,  he  took  them  off  again. 
"Pick  it  up,  brother!"  snapped  a  cop  as  the 
light  changed,  and  Mitty  hastily  pulled  on  his 
gloves  and  lurched  ahead.^ 


Others 


A— directs 


B— patronizes 

D— ridicules 
A— directs 
D— accuses 


A— directs 


I— obeys 

These  interactions  are  profiled  in  Figure  15. 

3  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty."  Copyright,  1939,  James  Thurber.  Originally 
published  in  The  New  Yorker. 


74 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


Figure  15.  Diagrammatic  Summary  of  Walter  Mitry  Interaction  at  Level  I-R.  Key: 
Radius  of  circle  =  4  raw  scores. 

These  profiles  indicate  that  submissioa  and  passive  resistance 
characterize  Walter  iMitty's  overt  behavior.  He  provokes  bossy,  su- 
perior, and  critical  behavior  from  others. 

The  underlying  feelings  of  Thurber's  hero  are,  of  course,  quite  dif- 
ferent. There  are  five  fantasies  included  in  this  story  each  of  which 
portrays  the  hero  as  commanding,  successful,  proud,  disdainful,  and 
deeply  respected. 


Level  Ill-F  of  Walter  Mitty  as  Scored  by  the  Interpersonal  System 
(Molecular  Rating) 


Self 
A— commands 


"We're  going  through!"  The  Commander's 
voice  was  like  thin  ice  breaking.  He  wore  his 
full-dress  uniform,  with  the  heavily  braided  white 
cap  pulled  down  rakishly  over  one  cold  gray  eye. 
"We  can't  make  it,  sir.  It's  spoiling  for  a  hur- 
D— stem  firmness  ricane,  if  you  ask  me."  "I'm  not  asking  you, 
Lieutenant  Berg,"  said  the  Commander.  "Throw 
on  the  power  hghts!  Rev  her  up  to  8,500!  We're 
going  through!"  The  pounding  of  the  cylinders 
increased :  ta-pocketa-pocketa-pocketa-poc^era- 
pocketa.  The  Commander  stared  at  the  ice  form- 
ing on  the  pilot  window.  He  walked  over  and 
twisted  a  row  of  complicated  dials.  "Switch  on 
No.  8  auxiUary!"  he  shouted.  "Switch  on  No.  8 
auxiliary!"  repeated  Lieutenant  Berg.  "Full 
strength  in  No.  3  turret!"  The  crew,  bending 
to  their  various  tasks  in  the  huge,  hurtling  eight- 
engined  Navy  hydroplane,  looked  at  each  other 
and  grinned.  "The  Old  Man'll  get  us  through," 
thev  said  to  one  another.  "The  Old  Man  ain't 
afraid  of  HeU!"  .  .  .=» 
Ibid. 


B— confidence 


A— commands 
B— confidence 


A— commands 


A—coTnmands 


Others 


F— complain 


I— obeys 
I— obeys 


]— admire 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  175 

The  item  by  item  scoring  of  each  interpersonal  action  in  this  fantasy 
illustrates  the  molecular  system  for  rating  Level  III-F  behavior. 
Where  several  fantasies  are  available  it  is  often  economical  to  assign  a 
single  summary  rating  to  the  behavior  of  the  hero  and  other.  This  is 
called  molar  rating.  The  molar  scores  for  this  episode  would  be: 

Mitty  Others 

AB  1} 

This  scoring  translates  into  the  verbal  summary:  "The  hero  is  com- 
manding and  self-confident;  others  obey  and  admire  him." 

The  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Symbols 

from  the  The?natic  Apperception  Test 

In  the  Thematic  Apperception  Test  (8)  the  subject  is  given  a  series 
of  cards  which  contain  magazine-type  illustrations.  The  task  is  to  tell 
a  story  which  fits  the  picture.  He  is  requested  to  describe  the  action, 
the  feelings  of  the  characters,  and  the  outcome  of  the  plot. 

The  following  illustrative  story  was  told  in  response  to  TAT  Card 
2-i  which  pictures  a  farm  scene,  a  young  woman  in  the  foreground 
holding  books,  an  older  woman  in  the  background  leaning  against 
a  tree,  and  a  man  in  the  distance  plowing.  A  patient  told  the  following 
story  of  this  picture.  The  scored  themes  are  italicized: 

Symbolic  Symbolic 

Self  (Hero)  Other 

Scores  Scores 

They  probably  got  up  and  had  breakfast— the  Mother 

and  Daughter. 
The  daughter  looks  like  she  hates  her  Mother.  £-3 

I  guess  because  of  the  Mother's  stern  look.  D-2 

Her  body  shows  no  emotion.  I  think  she'll  run  away  H-1 

—but  not  from  the  school— because  she  resents  her  F-3 

Mother  who  is  so  unkind  to  her  and  D-3 

doesn't  show  her  any  love.  C-2 

My  own  feeling  tells  me  she  flees  from  the  situation.  H-3 

It  will  be  noted  that  every  interpersonal  feeling  or  action  in  this 
story  was  given  a  separate  score.  This  method  of  rating  every  inter- 
personal unit  is  called  molecular  scoring — every  interpersonal  detail 
gets  a  separate  rating.  It  is  possible  to  add  up  all  the  scores  given  to 
the  hero  and  to  the  "other"  in  this  story — or  to  summarize  all  of  the 
molecular  themes  from  all  the  TAT  cards. 

The  scores  for  this  particular  story,  combined  into  a  Level  III-T 
profile,  are  shown  in  Figure  16.  This  graphically  portrays  the  themes 
of  anger  (£),  resentment  (F),  and  retreat  (H)  from  a  hostile  (D) 
and  rejecting  (C)  parent. 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


SYMBOLIC   HERO    ( YOUNG   WOMAN) 


SYMBOLIC   OTHER    ( MOTHER) 


Figure  16.  Diagrammatic  Representation  of  Interpersonal  Scores  for  an  Illustrative 
TAT  Story.  Key:  Radius  of  octants  of  circle  =  4  scores. 

Another  method  for  scoring  TAT  data  which  has  proved  more 
convenient  is  to  read  over  the  entire  story  and  to  assign  an  over-all 
score  to  the  basic  relationship  between  hero  and  other.  This  is  called 
summary  or  molar  TAT  scoring.  For  this  particular  story  the  molar 
rating  would  be: 


Hero  (Daughter) 


Other  (Mother) 


This  summary  formula  tells  us  that  the  fantasy  hero  resents  and  then 
withdraws  from  an  unsympathetic  mother. 

Case  SumTnaries  Illustrating  the  Relationship 
Principle  of  Symbolic  Data 

At  this  point  we  have  just  about  completed  the  description  of  three 
of  the  four  levels  of  personality  which  are  now  employed  by  the 
Kaiser  Foundation  project.  It  is  now  possible  to  put  the  data  for  the 
three  levels  together  and  to  work  out  some  tentative  formulations 
about  some  of  the  relationships  among  levels.  The  following  case 
studies  are  designed  to  show  that  symbolic  data  are  useless  when 
studied  in  isolation  and  that  they  are  most  useful  when  studied  in  re- 
lationship to  other  levels  of  personality. 

Symbols  of  Distrust,  Deprivation,  and  Isolation.  As  part  of  the 
initial  evaluation  process  at  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychiatric  Clinic 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  177 

patients  are  given  a  battery  of  tests  which  measure  different  levels  of 
the  interpersonal  system.  When  the  tests  from  each  level  are  scored, 
the  results  are  fitted  together  to  determine  the  interpersonal  diagnosis 
and  to  provide  the  data  for  clinical  prediction. 

Let  us  consider  a  subject  whose  "preconscious"  language  is  loaded 
with  themes  of  deprivation,  distrust,  and  loneliness.  One  such  patient 
described  all  his  heroes  as  failures,  unloved,  beaten,  and  exploited. 
They  all  ended  up  suicidal,  isolated,  frustrated.  All  his  endings  were 
unhappy. 

With  these  data  in  mind,  what  predictions  can  we  make  about  his 
conscious  view  of  self  or  his  interpersonal  reflexes?  Many  such  pa- 
tients see  themselves  consciously  in  the  same  way.  They  report  their 
real  life  as  being  frustrated,  unhappy,  isolated.  They  may  describe 
their  interpersonal  world  as  being  reciprocally  rejecting  and  disap- 
pointing. In  Figure  17  we  see  such  a  patient.  The  subject's  conscious 
description  matches  the  discouragement  of  his  symbols.  The  fantasied 
others  are  unsympathetic,  much  as  he  describes  his  own  father. 

Many  other  subjects  whose  symbolic  heroes  are  deprived  and  iso- 
lated show  a  markedly  different  pattern  at  Level  II-C.  The  self  de- 
scriptions, instead  of  being  weak,  isolated,  and  pessimistic,  may  empha- 
size strength  and  success.  A  clear  conflict  exists  between  the  self- 
perception  and  the  self  as  symbolized. 

The  meaning  of  the  fantasy  productions  varies  considerably  in  these 
two  cases.  The  passive  deprivation  means  one  thing  when  it  dupli- 
cates the  pessimism  of  Levels  II  and  I.  It  means  another  thing  in  the 
context  of  overt  overoptimism  and  expansiveness.  A  diagnosis  of  con- 
flict is  defined  in  the  latter  case.  If  this  conflicting  pattern  is  confirmed 
by  the  other  system  measurements  and  by  the  clinical  history,  a  con- 
siderably different  prognosis  and  therapeutic  program  would  result. 

Level  III  takes  on  meaning  in  relationship  to  the  other  levels  of  per- 
sonality. 

Conflicting  Symbolic  Themes  of  Power  and  Weakness.  The 
two  cases  just  considered  possessed  rather  narrow  symbolic  patterns 
emphasizing  the  themes  FGHI.  This  overloading  of  symbolic  themes 
in  one  direction  is  not  unusual — neither  is  it  inevitable.  Many  patients 
reflect  a  more  ambivalent  picture  at  the  symbolic  level.  In  these  cases 
the  fantasy  material  may  indicate  the  nature  of  an  underlying  conflict 
— but  they  do  not  tell  us  which  side  of  the  ambivalence  is  being  ex- 
pressed at  the  overt  levels.  They  do  not  tell  which  interpersonal  re- 
flex pattern  is  employed. 

Let  us  take  as  example  a  patient  whose  TAT  stories  were  concerned 
with  the  themes  of  strength  and  weakness.  In  some  of  his  fantasies  the 


78 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


(Hi) 
LEVEL   II-C    SELF 


LEVEL   II-C   FATHER 


LEVEL   III-T   HERO 


LEVEL   III-T   OTHER 


Figure  17.  Level  II-C  and  Level  III-T  Hero  and  Other  Profiles  for  Illustrative  Case. 
Key:  Radius  of  Level  III-T  circles  =  8  scores;  radius  of  Level  II-C  circles  =  16  scores. 
Raw  scores  are  employed  in  these  illustrative  diagrams. 

heroes  were  wise  and  respected  figures  winning  the  attention  of  ad- 
miring followers.  In  other  stories  the  heroes  were  docile,  dependent 
figures  looking  up  to  powerful,  esteemed  others.  All  the  people  de- 
scribed in  his  imagination  were  either  leading  or  being  led.  They 
diagrammed  in  Figure  1 8 : 

These  symbols  clearly  reveal  a  preoccupation  with  strength  and 
weakness.  They  suggest  that  a  marked  ambivalence  exists  concerning 
power  motives.  With  this  knowledge  of  the  subject's  fantasy  con- 
flict can  we  diagnose  the  other  levels  of  personality.^  Not  very  well. 
We  can  safely  guess  that  the  overt  levels  of  behavior  will  fall  along 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


179 


SYMBOLIC  HERO 


SYMBOLIC   OTHERS 


Figure  18.  Diagrammatic  Representation  of  Symbolic  Hero  and  "Other"  Scores 
Illustrating  Conflict  Between  Power  and  Weakness.  Key:  Radius  of  each  octant  of 
circle  =  8  scores  (i.e.,  raw  TAT  ratings). 

the  power-submission  axis  {AP  versus  H,  I,  J,  K).  It  is  statistically 
unlikely  that  he  will  describe  himself  or  present  himself  as  hostile  or 
affectionate.  We  can  thus  eliminate  roughly  one  half  of  the  circular 
continuum  (D,  E,  F,  G  and  N,  O,  L,M). 

What  we  cannot  do  is  predict  whether  he  will  present  himself  as 
strong  or  weak.  His  interpersonal  reflexes  may  reflect  either  aspect 
of  the  underlying  conflict.  They  may  express  both  sides.  When  a 
conflict  between  dominance  and  submission  exists  at  the  "precon- 
scious"  level,  about  one  half  of  such  cases  manifest  themselves  as  strong 
people  denying  weakness.  Such  patients  resist  psychotherapy,  com- 
plain of  physical  rather  than  emotional  symptoms,  deny  emotional  dis- 
turbance, depression,  or  passivity.  These  patients  express  one  side  of 
their  ambivalence— the  strong  side.  They  "sit  on"  the  weak  aspects. 
These  patients  are  often  called  counterphobic  or  compensatory  cases. 
This  means  that  they  react  against  their  underlying  feelings  of  weak- 
ness by  appearing  very  strong,  indeed.  Figure  19  shows  how  one  such 
patient  appeared  at  Levels  I-S  and  II-C. 

But  many  other  patients  with  the  same  TAT  pattern  of  conflicting 
strength  and  weakness  appear  quite  differently  at  the  other  levels. 
They  stress  the  passivity  side  of  the  power-passivity  axis.  They  claim 
to  be  depressed,  immobilized,  inferior  people.  They  apply  for  psycho- 
therapy eagerly.  They  have  strongly  developed  reflexes  of  helpless- 
ness. They  willingly  admit  their  need  for  treatment.  These  people 
are  also  "sitting  on"  one  side  of  their  underlying  conflict— the  strong 
side.  The  fantasy  themes  of  power  and  esteem  are  consciously  attrib- 


i8o 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


LEVEL   I-S   SELF;    INTERPERSONAL 
REFLEXES    AS   OBSERVED    BY    OTHERS 


LEVEL   II-C   SELF;   CONSCIOUS 
VIEW   OF   SELF 


Figure  19.  Diagrammatic  Representation  of  Level  I-S  &  II-C  Self  Scores  Illustrating 
a  Facade  of  Power  and  Responsibility. 

u ted  to  Others  (often  the  therapist) .  The  strong-dominant  side  of  their 
fantasy  coin  which  does  not  overtly  appear  usually  manifests  itself  in 
the  later  sequences  of  the  relationship.  Patients  who  present  them- 
selves in  this  manner  are  commonly  called  by  several  names  (usually 
depending  on  the  specific,  peripheral  aspects  of  symptomology) . 
They  include  the  phobics,  neurasthenics,  anxiety  neurotics.  Figure  20 
shows  how  a  typical  case  might  profile. 


'  (Hi 

LEVEL   I-S   SELF:    INTERPERSONAL 
REFLEXES   AS   OBSERVED   BY   OTHERS 


LEVEL   II-C  SELF:    CONSCIOUS 
VIEW    OF    SELF 


Figure  20.  Diagrammatic  Representation  of  Level  I-S  &  II-C  Self  Scores  Illustrating 
a  Fagade  of  Weakness  and  Docility. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  i8i 

Two  theoretical  points  have  been  developed  in  this  last  series  of  il- 
lustrations— one  old,  the  other  new.  The  first  is  the  familiar  refrain 
— the  language  of  symbols  does  not  necessarily  duplicate  or  reverse  the 
other  levels  of  personality  structure.  In  his  overt  behavior  the  patient 
may  repeat  the  symbolic  motifs — or  he  may  be  counterbalanced  away 
from  them.  Symbols  often  predict  future  change  in  overt  behavior 
— but  their  meaning  must  always  be  assessed  in  terms  of  the  total  per- 
sonality structure. 

The  second  point  concerns  the  notion  of  the  conflict  axis.  We  have 
noted  that  symbols  can  be  the  same  or  they  can  be  different  from  the 
levels  of  reflex  action  and  perception.  They  can  also  be  mixtures — 
combining  the  overt  motives  with  the  new  themes  unique  to  the  sym- 
bolic language.  In  these  cases  symbolic  ambivalence  is  present.  The 
imaginative  themes  cluster  into  two  polar  areas.  One  of  these  is 
usually  stressed  overtly  and  one  is  not. 

When  this  pattern  of  scores  develops,  a  conflict  is  defined.  The 
kind  of  conflict  is  determined  by  the  location  of  the  thematic  clusters 
around  the  circle.  The  last  case  presentation  illustrated  a  phobic- 
counterphobic  conflict  in  which  docile  weakness  covered  underlying 
conflict  between  strength  and  weakness. 

When  a  patient's  multilevel  pattern  of  scores  tends  to  cluster  into 
two  areas  in  the  diagnostic  circle  we  speak  of  the  conflict  axis.  In 
the  last  two  illustrations  of  phobic-counterphobic  behavior  the  conflict 
axis  was  I- A.  Other  common  dichotomous  clusters  are  D-H  which 
defines  sado-masochistic  conflict;  O-K  denoting  nurturance-depend- 
ence;  M-E  denoting  love-hate,  etc.  The  relation  between  fantasy 
heroes  and  their  protagonists  often  defines  such  reciprocal  clusters.  In 
one  story  the  disappointed  hero  is  rejected  by  the  exploitive  lover.  In 
the  next  story  the  hero  spurns  his  heartbroken  mate  to  follow  his  own 
selfish  goals.  In  the  language  of  the  notational  system  these  are  ex- 
pressed as: 

1 )  Hero  Lover 

G  C 

2)  Hero  Mate 

C  G 

A  conflict  of  rejecting  versus  being  rejected  is  thus  suggested.  The 
conflict  axis  is  C-G.  The  level  of  symbolism  can  help  define  the  con- 
flict axis — the  focal  centers  around  which  the  patient's  behavior  at 
overt  levels  tends  to  cluster.  Symbols  can  predict  basic  multilevel 
conflicts. 

Sweet  Symbols  Behind  a  Facade  of  Sweetness.  In  the  last  few 
pages  we  have  used  some  tentative  case  illustrations  to  fit  together  data 


,82  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

from  three  levels  of  personality.  These  have  served  to  underline  some 
earlier  principles  and  to  introduce  some  new  concepts  (such  as  the 
conflict  axis).  We  turn  now  to  a  third  type  of  case  which  will  con- 
tinue this  process  of  review  and  preview. 

The  subject  in  this  case  produced  fantasies  that  are  models  of 
"sweetness  and  light."  The  heroes  are  affectionate,  conventional,  and 
generous.  When  evil  or  hatred  appears  it  is  met  by  virtue  and  agree- 
abihty — and  usually  transformed  into  good.  All  the  endings  are  happy. 
In  the  case  of  the  TAT  even  those  cards  which  are  loaded  with  nega- 
tive stimuli  are  transferred  into  positive.  One  card  is  seen  by  most 
people  as  portraying  a  girl  slumped  beside  a  gun.  The  case  in  ques- 
tion sees  this  as  a  girl  overcome  with  joy,  the  vague  gun-like  object 
beside  her  becomes  a  "gift"  from  a  loved  one  which  has  led  to  the 
joyous  collapse.  Another  card  which  pictures  a  sprawled-out  figure 
described  by  most  people  as  dead  or  wounded  is  seen  by  our  subject 
as  a  "sleeping  man  exhausted  from  a  day  of  good  work."  Figure  21 
shows  how  the  symbolic  scores  might  profile. 

The  scores  for  Level  II-C  self  and  father  are  also  included,  showing 
that  the  pollyanna  pattern  of  optimism  and  goodness  appears  at  the 
level  of  conscious  description.  Now  it  is  quite  possible  to  have  siveet 
symbolic  themes  and  hostile  self-perceptions.  When  this  occurs  we 
have  a  conflict  between  a  rough  exterior  and  a  symbolic  heart  of  gold. 
The  case  profiled  in  Figure  2 1  shows  no  conflict,  however.  A  sweet 
and  loving  fagade  covers  sweet  and  loving  symbols. 

This  means  that  the  subject  tells  us — in  the  language  of  fantasy — 
the  same  message  that  he  has  expressed  in  conscious  description.  He 
denies  hostility  at  Level  II-C  and  both  "layers"  of  Level  III-T.  We 
have  assumed  that  a  defensive  process  leads  to  extreme  avoidance  of 
negative  affect  in  conscious  description.  The  same  process  spills  over 
into  the  level  of  imagination.  We  call  this  phenomenon  a  rigid  in- 
varicmce.  We  conclude  that  the  same  anxiety  process  which  imbalanced 
Level  II-C  in  the  direction  of  socially  approved  motives  is  at  work  at 
Level  III-T.  We  suspect  that  hostility  and  assertive,  bitter  feelings 
are  so  anxiety  provoking  that  they  cannot  be  expressed  in  the  more 
flexible  language  of  symbolism.  They  cannot  appear  in  responses  to 
the  cards  on  which  the  average  person  reports  them.  They  cannot 
even  be  projected  onto  the  fantasy  "others."  Even  if  the  stimulus 
picture  on  the  card  pulls  for  hostile  themes,  this  subject  can  maneuver 
his  perceptions  to  avoid  them.  Ravaged  corpses  become  sleeping 
beauties. 

Three  points  are  worth  stressing — the  first  two  are  familiar  and  the 
other  is  new.  The  first:  fantasy  themes  give  a  rough  indication  of  the 
interpersonal  source  of  anxiety  and  the  amount  of  it.   If  the  themes 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


[83 


LEVEL   II-C   CONSCIOUS 
VIEW   OF   SELF 


LEVEL   II-C   CONSCIOUS 
VIEVi^   OF   FATHER 


'(Hli 

LEVEL    III-T    HERO 


(Hi) 

LEVEL   ni-T   OTHER 


Figure  21.  Level  II-C  &  Level  III-T  Hero  &  Other  Profiles  for  a  Rigidly  Conven- 
tional Patient. 

that  are  strictly  avoided  at  Levels  I  and  II  are  also  eschevv^ed  at  Level 
III  we  may  speculate  that  these  themes  are  the  source  of  anxiety  for 
this  patient.  The  consistency  of  the  avoidance  at  all  levels  indicates 
roughly  how  much  anxiety.  In  the  illustrated  case  we  might  guess 
that  there  is  plenty  of  anxiety  connected  with  the  expression  of  ag- 
gression and  antisocial  motifs. 

Symbolic  Themes  Are  Not  Always  'Tre conscious'' 
or  Opposed  to  Consciousness 
A  second  issue  illustrated  by  this  case  concerns  the  nature  of  fantasy 
expressions.  The  fact  that  themes  appear  in  dreams  or  projective  tests 


I  §4  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

does  not  mean  that  they  are  necessarily  different  from  conscious  per- 
ceptions. Autistic  productions  are  not  an  automatic  "pipehne"  to  the 
underlying  motivation.  They  can  simply  repeat  the  pattern  of  secur- 
ity operations  employed  at  the  overt  levels. 

This  point  has  not  been  made  clear  in  the  psychological  literature. 
There  is  a  common  tendency  to  assume  that  symbolic  or  projective 
data  inevitably  denote  repressed  or  unconscious  material.  An  amus- 
ing illustration  of  this  fallacious  assumption  concerns  the  symbolic 
expressions  of  Nazi  leaders  who  were  given  personality  tests  while 
awaiting  trial  in  Nuremburg.  These  findings  have  been  described  in 
an  excellent  book  by  Dr.  Gilbert,  the  prison  psychologist  ( 3 ) .  One  of 
those  tested  was  Colonel  Hoess,  the  S.  S.  official  in  charge  of  the 
Auschwitz  concentration  camp.  It  has  been  estimated  that  this  man 
was  directly  responsible  for  the  deaths  of  over  two  million  prisoners. 
As  such  he  probably  ranks  among  the  most  murderous  and  sadistic 
human  beings  who  have  ever  lived.  A  book  reviewer  for  a  psycho- 
logical journal  noted  w^ith  surprise  that  the  fantasy  test  expressions  of 
Colonel  Hoess  were  loaded  with  savage,  cruel  hostility.  This  led  the 
reviewer  to  wonder  why  Colonel  Hoess  would  have  repressed  sadism 
in  his  TAT  stories  when  he  overtly  acted  out  so  much  aggression  in 
his  behavior. 

It  seems  clear  that  this  psychologist  was  erroneously  equating 
fantasy  productions  with  repressed  or  unconscious  material.  The 
theory  of  variability  developed  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research 
(see  Chapter  13)  would  expect  that  a  person  who  expresses  such  in- 
tense hostility  and  who  rigidly  avoids  tender,  humanitarian  feelings  at 
Levels  I  and  II  would  probably  be  unable  to  tolerate  positive  feelings 
even  at  the  level  of  symbolism.  Colonel  Hoess,  we  suspect,  was  least 
anxious  when  he  was  employing  cold,  sadistic  security  operations.  It 
is  not  surprising  that  the  same  avoidance  of  affiliative  emotions  re- 
appeared at  Level  III-T.  Three-layer  expressions  of  the  same  theme 
are  typical  of  chronic  maladjusted  characters. 

Variation  in  Depth  of  Symbol  Instruweiits 

The  third  issue  evolving  from  this  sample  case  concerns  the  defini- 
tion of  levels  and  sublevels.  At  Level  II  we  obtain  the  picture  that  the 
subject  wishes  to  present  to  us — his  conscious  reports.  At  Level  III  he 
communicates  in  an  indirect  language  that  need  not  be  bound  by  the 
limits  of  the  real  world.  We  have  assumed  thap  Level  III  is  related  to 
the  private  world.  x\ll  human  beings  have  a  world  of  mental  reserva- 
tions which  are  more  or  less  distinct  from  what  they  directly  express. 
The  different  layers  of  symbolic  behavior,  it  seems,  come  closest  to 
expressing  these. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  185 

But  in  many  cases  Level  III  hero  and  other  scores  are  the  same  as 
Level  IL  The  same  themes  are  emphasized  or  avoided  at  both  levels. 
The  bland  hysterical  personality  just  presented  was  one  such  case. 
The  sadistic  Nazi  executioner  whose  symbols  were  hostile  is  another 
such  case.  Still  other  patients  stress  pessimistic  themes.  They  are  de- 
pressed and  bitter  at  Level  II  and  an  equally  unhappy  blackness  stains 
their  symbols.  We  surmise  that  trustful,  tender  emotions  are  so  threat- 
ening to  these  patients  that  they  must  avoid  them  even  in  fantasies. 

When  the  autisms  of  Level  III  are  the  same  as  the  conscious  descrip- 
tions of  Level  II,  a  puzzHng  question  occurs.  In  this  event  it  would 
seem  that  the  private  language  is  the  same  as  the  overt  and  conscious. 
This  is  faintly  paradoxical.  A  confusion  of  levels  is  suggested.  Since 
the  private  is  also  public,  it  suggests  either  that  these  patients  have  no 
thoughts  that  are  exclusively  private  (i.e.,  secret)  or  that  the  symbols 
have  failed  to  express  the  private.  If  the  first  conjecture  is  true,  then 
the  subject  is  unusually  frank  and  honest — having,  as  it  were,  no  men- 
tal reservation.  If  the  second  is  true,  then  the  subject  is  unusually 
repressive  and  secretive — he  succeeds  in  blanketing  his  symbols  with 
the  same  avoidance  tactics  that  characterize  his  conscious  expressions. 
The  former  would  be  bluntness  and  insight.  The  latter  would  be 
evasion  and  symbolic  denial. 

According  to  the  theory  developed  in  this  chapter,  if  symbols  dupli- 
cate the  extreme  imbalances  of  the  conscious  level,  then  considerable 
information  about  the  patient's  anxiety  system  is  available.  The  sub- 
ject compulsively  avoids  hostihty  in  consciousness.  Even  in  symbol 
he  cannot  tolerate  an  expression  of  the  negative.  If  he  does  have 
private  feelings  of  bitterness  or  aggression,  they  are  not  allowed  sym- 
bolic expression.  He  does  not  dare  let  his  symbols  express  his  private 
feehngs.  Level  III  instruments  (e.g.,  TAT  tests)  are,  in  this  case,  tap- 
ping only  Level  II.  They  do  not  "dig  down  deep  enough."  A  very 
thick  layer  of  defensive  avoidance  exists — so  that  the  Level  III  instru- 
ments fail  to  get  at  the  private  world. 

For  some  patients  in  some  situations  projective  tests  such  as  the 
TAT  fail  to  reach  anything  different  from  conscious  report.  Consider 
a  patient  who  covers  feelings  of  distrust  and  depriv^ation  with  a  facade 
of  extroverted  congeniality.  If  he  takes  a  TAT  in  connection  with  ap- 
plying for  a  desirable  job,  the  underlying  feelings  may  not  appear  in 
his  fantasy  stories.  The  same  patient  applying  for  therapeutic  help  in 
the  psychiatric  clinic  may  produce  TAT  stories  which  express  his 
feelings  of  sorrow  and  defeat.  The  same  person,  were  he  attempting 
to  "buck  for  a  medical  discharge"  from  the  Army,  might  overexag- 
ger*ate  his  depressive  feelings  on  the  TAT — they  might  even  be  picked 
up  by  Level  II  instruments. 


,86  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

The  fact  that  we  obtain  Level  III  fantasy  protocols  does  not  mean 
that  we  are  necessarily  tapping  the  private  world  of  the  patient.  Meth- 
ods of  measuring  fantasy  vary  in  depth.  The  "preconscious"  self  in 
some  subjects  is  closer  to  consciousness  than  the  images  of  symbolic 
others.  Dreams  seem  to  produce  themes  which  are  most  distant  from 
conscious  report  and  thus  deeper.  To  use  Freudian  terminology,  some 
parts  of  some  dreams  seem  to  tap  the  primary  processes  characteristic 
of  schizophrenic  or  infantile  thought.  Projective  tests  and  fantasies 
are  generally  "preconscious"  and  probably  tap  secondary  processes. 

Level  III  instruments  might  be  compared  with  drilling  machines 
which  tap  geological  strata.  We  cannot  assume  that  the  TAT  or  a 
fantasy  automatically  taps  private  feelings.  The  depth  and  thickness 
of  the  strata  of  conscious  report  is  a  crucial  and  variable  factor.  As  il- 
lustrated in  Figure  22,  a  flexible  person  with  minimum  anxiety  (Case 
2)  may  confide  his  "preconscious"  feelings  in  interviews.  He  has  some 
conscious  awareness  of  his  ambivalences  and  is  able  to  discuss  them. 
When  the  conflict  is  more  severe  and  anxiety  greater  (Case  3)  more 
indirect  instruments  may  be  required  to  hit  the  private  "layers."  This 
type  of  patient  may  present  the  same  picture  in  a  check  list  and  even 
in  therapy  interviews.  The  TAT  themes  and  dreams  may  indicate  the 
other  side  of  the  ambivalence.  In  other  cases  (Case  4)  the  anxiety  ac- 
companying certain  interpersonal  emotions  is  so  great  that  they  do 
not  appear  in  the  expressions  of  the  "preconscious"  self.  The  absent 
themes  may  be  projected  on  the  "preconscious"  world  or  they  may 
appear  only  in  the  subliminal  expressions  of  Level  IV, 

Determining  the  Depth  of  the  Measuring  Instrument 

Use  of  a  Level  III  test  thus  does  not  guarantee  that  the  "precon- 
scious" will  be  discovered.  This  fact  does  not  in  any  way  lessen  the 
value  of  these  tests.  They  always  assist  in  determining  the  rigidity  and 
depth  of  the  defensive  processes.  A  vital  part  of  interpersonal  diag- 
nosis is  to  determine  the  amount  of  anxiety  and  the  way  it  operates  to 
inhibit  or  deny  certain  touchy  emotions. 

The  interpretation  of  a  projective  test  or  dream  is  greatly  facili- 
tated if  we  know  the  depth  of  the  private  world  and  the  "thickness" 
of  the  conscious  defensive  processes.  These  are  determined  in  two 
ways. 

As  soon  as  the  data  are  obtained  from  a  Level  III  test  we  compare 
them  with  the  data  from  Level  II.  If  the  symbols  clearly  duplicate  the 
rigidities  of  Level  II,  then  we  estimate  the  conscious  defensive  pro- 
cesses extended  down  to  the  depth  of  the  test.  The  more  rigid  the 
similarity  between  the  symbolic  instrument  and  Level  II — the  greater 
the  anxiety,  the  thicker  the  defensive  strata.  In  these  cases  the  thematic 


u 

6 

Oscillator: 

conflicts 

at  all 

levels 

^1 ,  §^ 

1  .' 

3 
C 

6 

f "  s  s 

B 

B 

p 

fill 

i 

R 

1 

lllti 

B 

hi" 

111 

!2 

z 

t 

z 

> 

> 

II"  i 

r\ 

o 

mi 

O 

5 

11 

ex  a. 

/^ 

u 

si 

r>j 

^ 

< 
Id 

> 

^Hl 

o 

^sii 

r>| 

^ 

S  A  E  ^ 

_r^ 

^J 

1 

c3--B| 

-o 

187 


,88  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

aspects  of  Level  III  add  nothing  new.  As  each  additional  depth  test 
repeats  the  same  themes,  we  learn  nothing  new  about  -what  themes  are 
present  in  the  elusive  warded-off  private  world — but  we  do  learn 
something  about  how  deep  it  is  and  how  much  anxiety  is  tied  to  it. 
The  first  way  of  determining  the  depth  of  the  measuring  instrument  is 
to  compare  its  themes  with  Level  IL  The  more  discrepancy,  the 
further  from  consciousness. 

A  second  method  for  determining  the  depth  of  the  measuring  rod 
involves  use  of  internal  cues  of  defensiveness.  The  two  best  internal 
cues  for  estimating  defensiveness  from  projective  tests  are  mispercep- 
tions  of  stimuli  and  avoidance  of  specific  themes.  These  issues  are  dis- 
cussed in  the  next  chapter. 

Situational  Relativity  of  Symbols 

Another  factor  which  exerts  strong  pressure  on  symbolic  expres- 
sions concerns  the  motivation  of  the  patient  in  the  particular  situation. 
Consider  a  subject  whose  private  feelings  concern  weakness  and  des- 
pair. Suppose  he  is  applying  for  a  job  which  entails  executive  responsi- 
bility, and  as  part  of  the  application  procedures  is  administered  a 
fantasy  test.  Since  the  job  situation  would  tend  to  motivate  the  ex- 
pression of  strength  and  assertion — the  expression  of  his  private  feel- 
ings would  be  threatening.  The  feelings  of  inferiority  and  depression 
would,  therefore,  not  be  likely  to  appear.  The  subject  might  really 
have  the  wishful  fantasy  of  retreating  from  the  demands  of  the  world 
into  an  isolated,  lonely,  passive  life.  He  might  secretly  yearn  to  be  a 
forest-fire  watcher  or  a  beachcomber.  But  if  the  employment  inter- 
viewer asks  him  projective  questions  about  his  hopes  for  himself,  he 
might  respond  with  the  wishful  statement:  "I  want  a  responsible, 
managerial  job  with  a  big  company."  We  are,  in  this  last  statement, 
obviously  not  tapping  the  symbolic  level.  Level  III  is  being  used  to 
support  the  overt  presentation. 

The  cultural  situation  in  this  example  was  the  "job  application." 
The  cultural  situation  we  are  concerned  with  in  this  book  is  the 
psychiatric  clinic.  We  are  attempting  to  develop  a  functional  diag- 
nostic system  which  will  lead  to  predictions  about  clinic  behavior. 
When  patients  come  to  a  psychiatric  clinic  for  diagnostic  evaluation 
they  vary  considerably  in  their  motivation.  Their  symbolic  produc- 
tions will  vary  considerably.  Many  patientis  sense  psychological 
evaluation  and  therapy  as  a  threat  to  their  imbalanced,  inflexible  ad- 
justments. These  patients  may  avoid  in  their  fantasy  tests  the  same 
themes  that  cause  them  anxiety  at  Levels  II  and  I.  Motivation  of  the 
patient,  as  well  as  the  source  and  amount  of  anxiety,  is  a  complicating 
factor  in  symbolic  interpretation. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION  189 

This  is  not  a  particularly  distressing  complication  to  the  psychol- 
ogist evaluating  the  patient.  Certainly  we  concede  that  motivation 
varies  from  patient  to  patient  and  these  variances  influence  the  Level 
III  material.  But  "amount  of  motivation,"  far  from  being  a  distracting 
irrelevant  factor,  is  actually  most  central  to  prognosis.  If  motivation 
effects  the  production  of  symbols,  then  symbols  can  help  estimate  the 
amount  and  kind  of  motivation.  They  can  help  plan  the  correct 
therapeutic  program  that  works  with  and  does  not  clash  headlong 
against  the  unique  defensive  set-up  of  the  particular  patient. 

Let  us  recall  the  hysterical  patient  (page  181)  who  presented  sweet 
symbols  behind  a  facade  of  sweetness.  The  TAT  themes  duplicated 
the  conventional,  pious  traits  of  Level  II.  This  suggests  that  the  de- 
fensive structure  is  "thick" — the  anxiety  accompanying  antisocial  or 
negative  feelings  is  very  high.  The  patient  might  be  panicked  by  being 
referred  to  psychotherapy.  He  might  react  with  a  severe  anxiety  at- 
tack. Most  likely,  he  would  react  by  increasing  the  bland  denial  of 
pathology.  Both  of  these  reactions  would  postpone  the  onset  of  effec- 
tive psychological  help. 

At  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychiatric  Clinic  a  patient  with  such  a 
hysterical  "normality  syndrome"  would  not  be  rushed  into  psycho- 
therapy. The  nature  of  treatment  might  be  explained  to  him.  The 
intake  worker  might  discuss  with  the  patient  (in  nontechnical  lan- 
guage) the  nature  of  his  current  adjustment — by  referring  to  the  pa- 
tient's own  claims  to  health  and  hypernormality.  This  is  done  sup- 
portively.  To  use  psychoanalytic  terminology,  it  is  done  "from  the 
side  of  the  ego."  The  rigid  claiming  of  goodness  is  not  attacked  as  a 
defense  but  might  be  praised  as  a  valuable  means  of  adjustment.  No 
speculation  is  made  about  underlying  motivation.  The  advantage  of 
living  with  the  present  adjustment  (and  the  symptoms  it  involves) 
might  be  discussed.  The  function  of  psychotherapy  as  possibly  lead- 
ing to  different  solutions  might  be  mentioned,  but  not  pushed.  The 
patient  is  offered  the  opportunity  to  return  to  the  chnic  at  any  later 
date  if  symptoms  worsen  or  if  he  feels  it  worthwhile  to  learn  more 
about  his  patterns  of  living  and  the  possibility  of  changing  them. 

This  process  might  be  called  "planting  the  seed."  The  TAT,  in  this 
case,  provides  the  information  that,  at  present,  this  patient  is  deeply 
committed  to  hysterical  bland  techniques  of  adjustment.  Even  in 
fantasy  these  motives  appear.  The  TAT  predicts  that  exploration  of 
other  feelings  is,  at  present,  not  likely.  It  predicts  that  the  patient  in 
the  immediate  future  is  moving  away  from  and  not  toward  his 
warded-off  feelings. 

Level  III  thus  has  many  sublevels.  Some  of  these  are  determined  by 
the  nature  of  the  measuring  instrument.    Dreams  appear  to  be  the 


190 


i-J      !-? 


h    i 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


SqOv   —   O        5'000        QC30 


{EuuoiuadXH       >  2:  2  iQ  2 
-ajqisuodsa-a    <   "•  ^  ""  '^ 


g       I 


2        ;3 


o 


-SupBij3-jps 


1;  —     - 


—  Q—  rM-H  ,-1   ru   '-•   '"       r-j—*'— 


tj    H  onsisspjBf^       Cj  '^  00 


aAiiuadiuo^ 


ft-    C 


R    O  '5  '2  '"'      .=;   "   « 


THE  LEVEL  OF  PRIVATE  PERCEPTION 


[91 


deepest  sublevel.  Wishful  fantasies  and  projective  tests  are  less  deep. 
These  sublevels  vary  from  situation  to  situation.  In  six  months  the 
patient  described  here  may  return  to  the  clinic.  He  may  see  it  this 
time,  not  as  a  threatening  institution  ready  to  expose  his  antisocial 
feelings.  He  may  be  less  defensive.  His  thick  protective  strata  (which 
are  measured  by  the  extent  to  which  Level  II  themes  penetrate  and 
duplicate  Level  III  data)  may  be  considerably  diminished.  His  sec- 
ond testing  battery  may  show  the  same  conscious  description  but  a 
more  changeable  TAT.  He  would  then  be  considered  more  ready  to 
deal  with  his  conflict  between  bland  sweetness  and  the  underlying  feel- 
ings. 

Incidence  of  Level  lll-T  Behavior  in  Various  Cultural  Samples 

Detailed  summaries  of  the  research  findings  involving  Level  III 
behavior  are  presented  in  the  clinical  chapters  and  Appendix  3.  At  this 
point  the  percentage  of  Level  III-T  types  found  in  several  sympto- 
matic and  institutional  samples  are  presented  in  Table  5. 

It  will  be  observed  that  psychotics  manifest  "preconscious"  sadism 
(DE)  and  distrust  (FG);  medical  controls  (normals),  underlying 
power  (AP)  and  narcissism  (BC);  obese  patients,  power  (AP)  nar- 
cissism, and  hostility;  ulcer  patients,  an  intense  amount  of  underlying 
dependence  (JK),  etc. 

Significance  tests  and  discussion  of  the  clinical  and  theoretical  im- 
plications are  presented  in  Chapter  24. 

References 

1.  Blum,  G.  S.  The  Blacky  Pictures:  a  technique  for  the  exploration  of  personality 
dynamics.   New  York:  The  Psychological  Corporation,  1950. 

2.  Freud,  S.  The  basic  writings  of  Sigmund  Freud.  New  York:  Modern  Library, 
1938.  Copyright,  New  York:  The  Macmillan  Co. 

3.  Gilbert,  G.  M.  Psychology  of  dictatorship.  New  York:  The  Ronald  Press  Co., 
1950. 

4.  Iflund,  B.  Selective  recall  of  meaningful  materials  as  related  to  psychoanalytic 
formulations  in  certain  psychiatric  syndromes.  Unpublished  doctor's  dissertation. 
University  of  California,  Berkeley,  1953. 

5.  Kluckhohn,  C,  and  H.  A.  Murray.  Personality  in  nature,  society  and  culture. 
New  York:  Alfred  A.  Knopf,  Inc.,  1949. 

6.  Kris,  E.  On  preconscious  mental  processes.  Psychanal.  Quart.,  1950, 19,  pp.  540-60. 

7.  Langer,  S.  Philosophy  in  a  new  key.  Cambridge:  Harvard  University  Press,  1942. 

8.  Murray,  H.  A.  Thematic  Apperception  Test.  Cambridge:  Harvard  University 
Press,  1943. 

9.  Powelson,  D.,  and  R.  Bendix.  Psychiatry  in  prison.  Psychiat.,  1951,  14,  73-86. 

10.  Thurber,  J.  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty."  Copyright,  1939,  James  Thurber. 
Originally  published  in  The  New  Yorker. 

11.  ToMKiNs,  S.  S.  The  Thematic  Apperception  Test.  New  York:  Grune  &  Strat- 
ton,  1947. 


10 

The  Level  of  the  Unexpressed: 
Significant  Omissions' 


The  levels  of  personality  described  in  the  last  three  chapters  have 
moved  steadily  from  overt  observable  behavior  (Level  I)  through 
conscious  description  (Level  II)  into  the  two  private  or  underlying 
layers  of  the  "preconscious"  (Level  III  Hero  and  Other).  This  se- 
quential progression  frpm  the  external  to  the  internal  brings  us  now 
to  the  deepest  level  of  personality — Level  IV. 

This  is  called  the  level  of  the  unexpressed.  It  comprises  those  inter- 
personal themes  which  the  patient  consistently,  significantly,  and  spe- 
cifically omits  in  the  three  other  levels. 

The  Two  Criteria  for  Defining  Level  IV  Themes 

The  essence  of  Level  IV  themes  is  that  they  not  be  expressed  in  ac- 
tion, in  consciousness,  nor  in  the  "preconscious."  The  first  criterion  is 
that  the  themes  be  avoided  at  these  levels.  This  negative  evidence  can- 
not in  itself  be  taken  as  proof  that  the  themes  are  "dynamically"  ab- 
sent nor  that  they  exist  in  deeper  strata  of  the  personality.  To  accept 
this  absence  at  one  level  as  a  sign  of  presence  at  another  level  is  to  com- 
mit the  ancient  fallacy  of  reversal  which  was  discussed  in  the  preced- 
ing chapter. 

The  second  criterion  for  defining  Level  IV  requires  evidence  that 
the  themes  are  actively  avoided.  It  must  be  demonstrated  that  the  sub- 
ject selectively  and  stubbornly  refuses  to  respond  to  these  themes 
when  they  are  appropriate  in  the  situation.  It  is  not  enough  to  report 
that  a  patient  fails  to  express  a  particular  cluster  of  themes — let  us  say 
competitive  hostility — at  the  three  top  levels  of  behavior.  In  addition, 

*  This  level  of  personality  has  not  been  studied  systematically  by  the  Kaiser  Foun- 
dation psychology  research  group.  The  definition  and  discussion  in  this  chapter  is 
tentative  and  suggestive.  Readers  who  are  interested  in  the  current  clinical  or  re- 
search applications  of  the  interpersonal  system  can  safely  omit  this  chapter. 

192 


THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  UNEXPRESSED 


193 


it  must  be  shown  that  he  has  been  exposed  to  situations  in  which  he  is 
naturally  or  consensually  expected  to  perceive,  react  to,  or  express 
these  themes  and  that  he  has  refused  to  do  so. 

This  level  of  personality  has  received  little  empirical  attention  and 
is  therefore  not  included  in  the  systematic  or  clinical  studies  described 
in  this  book.  This  chapter  will  present  a  definition  and  a  survey  of 
some  tentative,  unvalidated  techniques  for  measuring  Level  IV.  Al- 
though in  the  subsequent  chapters  no  reference  will  be  made  to  Level 
IV,  the  present  discussion  is  included  as  a  preliminary  description  of 
this  incomplete  aspect  of  the  interpersonal  system. 

From  the  functional  point  of  view  the  existence  of  unexpressed 
interpersonal  themes  seems  to  be  of  some  importance.  It  might  be 
argued  that  in  clinical  practice  we  are  interested,  not  in  the  absent 
motives,  but  in  the  strata  of  ego  functions  which  lie  above  them  and 
which  seem  to  be  organized  in  warding  them  off.  When  we  deal  with 
a  patient  who  presents  conventional,  bland  themes  at  Levels  I,  II,  and 
III,  our  diagnostic  attention  is  obviously  going  to  be  focused  on  these 
ego  processes.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is  useful  to  know  that  inter- 
personal themes  comprise  the  Level  IV  significant  omissions.  These 
themes  can  be  expected  to  be  anxiety-laden.  If  the  patient  is  con- 
fronted with  them,  panic  may  develop.  Level  IV  defines  the  "touchy 
spots"  most  vigorously  and  desperately  avoided. 

In  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  we  have  tended  to  concentrate 
on  the  three  more  overt  levels.  In  psychoanalytic  language  we  have 
been  attempting  to  develop  an  ego  psychology.  Research  is  now  being 
planned  which  will  investigate  some  of  these  aspects  of  the  signifi- 
cantly omitted. 

The  methods  being  considered  for  these  future  studies  will  now  be 
reviewed. 

The  Measurement  of  Unexpressed  Themes 

It  has  been  pointed  out  that  there  are  two  empirical  criteria  for  the 
measurement  of  Level  IV  themes.  They  must  negatively  be  demon- 
strated to  be  significantly  absent  at  the  three  top  levels;  they  must 
positively  be  demonstrated  to  be  actively  avoided.  Two  separate 
measures  of  Level  IV  are  therefore  available — the  omission  and  the 
avoidance  scores.  These  two  criteria  will  be  treated  separately. 

The  Measurement  of  Omission  of  Interpersonal  Themes. 
The  first  criterion  is  easily  measured.  The  scores  at  Levels  I,  II,  III 
Hero,  and  III  Other  are  examined  to  see  what  behaviors  are  consist- 
ently avoided.  This  can  be  done  by  inspection  of  the  profiles  or  by 
means  of  arithmetical  indices. 


194 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


Figure  23  presents  the  profiles  of  an  illustrative  patient  who  has 
clearly  failed  to  express  rebellion  or  unconventional  themes. 


'(HI) 

LEVEL  I-S  SELF 


(Hi) 
LEVEL  II-C  SELF 


LEVEL  III-T  HERO 


(Hi) 

LEVEL   III-T   OTHER 


Figure  23.  Profiles  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  at  Four  Top  Layers  of  Personality 
Illustrating  the  Avoidance  of  Rebellious  (FG)  Behavior. 

By  inspection  we  see  that  the  FG  and  DE  octants  are  conspicuously 
neglected.  The  Level  I  profile  is  obtained  from  sociometric  ratings  of 
other  group  therapy  patients.  It  will  be  observed  that  the  fellow  group 
members  did  not  use  any  FG  or  DE  words  to  describe  this  patient's 
impact  upon  them.  In  his  Level  II-C  self  description  he  completely 
denied  any  of  these  behaviors.  Even  his  fantasy  heroes  and  others 
fail  to  receive  any  of  these  motivations.  It  is  possible  to  determine 
by  glancing  at  these  four  interpersonal  profiles  that  there  is  a  con- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  UNEXPRESSED 


[95 


sistent  tendency  to  avoid  the  expression  of  resentful,  rebellious,  hostile 
themes. 

In  addition  to  the  diagnosis  of  Level  IV  omission  by  inspection  of 
the  profiles  it  is  also  possible  to  employ  mathematical  techniques. 
These  allow  the  Level  IV  omission  profile  to  be  plotted  in  terms  of  a 
single  summary  point.  These  are  determined  by  the  horizontal  and 
vertical  indices  just  as  in  the  case  of  the  three  overt  levels. 

There  are  many  ways  in  which  Level  IV  omission  scores  can  be 
calculated.  It  is  possible  to  study  at  each  of  the  four  more  overt  levels 
the  sectors  which  the  subject  neglects.  We  could  study  all  of  the  items 
on  the  interpersonal  check  list  not  used  by  the  fellow  group  members 
in  rating  the  patient.  These  could  be  treated  hke  the  "yes"  scores, 
converted  into  the  horizontal  and  vertical  factors  and  plotted.  This 
provides  an  omission-at-Level-I  score.  The  same  could  be  done  for 
the  interpersonal  check  list  self-description  at  Level  II. 

We  want,  however,  a  single  summary  score  which  will  give  the 
over-all  pattern  of  what  is  omitted  at  Levels  I,  II,  and  III.  A  single 
way  of  estimating  the  Level  IV  omission  score  is  to  take  the  standard 
score  indices  for  Levels  I,  II,  III  Hero,  and  III  Other  (which  are  used 
to  plot  the  diagnostic  people)  and  to  establish  the  mean  horizontal 
and  vertical  indices  for  these  four  scores. 

For  the  patient  profiled  in  Figure  23  the  scores  were: 


Vertical 

Standard 

Score 

Horizontal 

Standard 

Score 

Level  I-S 
Level  II-C 
Level  III-T  Hero 
Level  III-T  Other 

64 
62 
41 
60 

66 
70 
94 
85 

Total 

227 

315 

Mean  =  ^  =  57 

f  =  " 

These  two  mean  indices  provide  a  summary  of  the  themes  the  pa- 
tient has  expressed  at  these  four  layers  of  personality.  If  these  indices 
are  each  subtracted  from  1 00  they  give  a  summary  of  what  the  patient 
has  avoided  at  Levels  I,  II,  and  III.  The  mean  omission  scores  for  this 
patient  are  vertical  =43,  horizontal  =21  (see  Figure  24).  This  is 
located  in  the  FG  octant  and  indicates  that  this  patient  compulsively 
and  markedly  omits  rebellious,  unconventional,  and  bitter  behavior  in 
his  expressions  at  Levels  I,  II,  III  Hero,  and  III  Other. 

The  Measurement  of  Significant  Avoidance  of  Interpersonal 
Themes.    There  are  two  criteria  for  determining  significant  omis- 


96 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


.0><^AN^ 


s^^\ 


S."=^- 


Uap) 


Uj 


ir/c 


'^/ 


'^if. 


^^fA 


.< 

,^^\<\ 

>:i^^ 


\ 

^- 

\^ 

0 

■^ 

/l 

\ 

N 

\ 

/ 

•s 

•o___ 

— -V 

/ 

\ 

/ 

^ 

0            ,              20 

30 

«  0 

\ 

. 

'0 

«0 

" 

^ 

\ 

/ 

/ 

\ 

\ 

\  ' 

-^^ 

\ 

/ 

. 

I 

V- 

.^ 

\ 

H« 

/ 

Figure  24.  Diagnostic  Grid  Containing  Summary  Points  for  the  Four  Top  Layers 
of  Personality  and  the  Level  IV  Omission  Score  Calculated  by  Subtraction.  Key: 
Radius  of  diagnostic  grid  =  6  sigmas,  /  =:  Level  I-S;  S  =r  Level  II-C  description  of  self; 
H  =  Level  III-T  (Hero):  O  =  Level  III-T  (Other);  IV  =  Level  IV  omission  score. 


sions  at  Level  IV.  The  first  is  to  demonstrate  that  certain  themes  are 
consistently  omitted  at  the  upper  levels.  A  method  has  just  been  pre- 
sented for  determining  this  type  of  Level  IV  score. 

The  second  criterion  involves  the  demonstration  that  these  themes 
are  actively  avoided  by  the  patient  at  the  upper  levels.  The  omissioii 
score  just  calculated  for  Level  IV  indicates  that  the  patient  has 
"claimed"  or  "expressed,"  and  by  a  process  of  subtraction  we  obtain 
the  Level  IV  index.  The  avoidance  score  is  obtained  in  a  different 
manner. 

There  are  at  least  three  techniques  for  estimating  the  tendency  to 
avoid  themes  at  Levels  I,  II,  and  III.  The  first  of  these  is  based  on  sta- 
tistical procedures  and  estimates  the  tendency  to  fail  to  perceive  or 


THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  UNEXPRESSED  197 

express  interpersonal  themes  where,  consensually,  most  others  do  per- 
ceive or  express  them. 

It  is  possible  to  determine  for  each  interpersonal  test  item  at  each 
level  the  probability  of  its  being  expressed.  The  interpersonal  check 
list  employed  at  Levels  I  and  II  has  been  subjected  to  intensive  sta- 
tistical analysis.  The  percentage  of  the  clinic  sample  expected  to  re- 
spond to  any  particular  word  has  been  determined.  The  128  words 
on  the  check  list  have  been  classified  into  four  groups  along  an  inten- 
sity dimension  in  terms  of  its  probabiUty  of  occurrence.  Intensity  1 
includes  words  which  are  checked  as  "true-about-self"  by  approxi- 
mately 90  per  cent  of  psychiatric  clinic  patients.  There  are  minimal 
amounts  of  the  trait  which  almost  everyone  is  willing  to  attribute  to 
himself.  Intensity  2  includes  words  employed  by  50  to  90  per  cent  of 
the  clinic  population.  Intensity  3  employs  more  intense  themes  used 
by  relatively  fewer  (10-50  per  cent)  patients.  Intensity  4  employs  ex- 
treme loadings  of  the  theme  which  are  rarely  checked  by  clinic  pa- 
tients. Here  are  illustrations  of  check  list  items  at  the  four  intensities 
for  the  interpersonal  variable  D,  which  includes  themes  ranging  from 
appropriate  sternness  to  punitive  sadism. 

Intensity  1  (very  common,  expected):  Able  to  be  strict 

Intensity  2  (average-moderate):  Stern  but  fair 

Intensity  3  (fairly  intense):  Sarcastic 

Intensity  4  (rare-extreme):  Cruel  and  unkind 

Each  item  on  the  check  list  is  thus  weighted  in  terms  of  its  consensual 
or  average  usage.  If  a  patient  does  not  check  the  Intensity  1  word  for 
any  interpersonal  variable,  he  is  failing  to  attribute  to  himself  a  mild 
amount  of  this  theme  which  90  per  cent  of  the  clinic  population  does 
express.  In  determining  the  Level  II  diagnostic  indices,  the  weightings 
of  the  items  are  not  considered.  All  the  words  used  in  every  octant  are 
fed  into  the  formulas. 

To  determine  the  significant  avoidance  score  these  weights  are  of 
usefulness.  At  Levels  I  and  II  which  employ  the  check  list  we  can 
study  the  pattern  of  avoidance  of  Intensity  1  and  2  items.  Weights  can 
be  assigned  so  that  the  failure  to  check  these  mild,  average,  statistically 
common  items  about  the  self  can  be  cast  into  numerical  indices  of  sig- 
nificant avoidance. 

At  Level  III  (as  measured  by  the  TAT)  the  test  stimuli  are  pictures 
which  portray  human  beings  in  interaction.  The  probability  of  any 
given  interpersonal  theme  being  expressed  in  reaction  to  any  particular 
card  has  been  determined.  Thus  we  discover  that  85  per  cent  of  the 
clinic  population  respond  with  the  fantasy  theme  HI   (sorrow  or 


1 98  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

guilt)  on  card  3BM,  whereas  less  than  2  per  cent  will  produce  a  story 
involving  AP  (dominant  power).  This  information  makes  it  possible 
to  weigh  the  thematic  pull  of  every  card  on  the  TAT  (or  any  other 
fantasy  test).  Patients  who  consistently  resist  the  card  pull  for  any 
particular  interpersonal  theme  can  be  assigned  Level  IV  avoidance 
indices  which  are  based  on  the  percentage  expectance. 

This  general  method  for  measuring  significant  omission  has  been 
previously  described  by  William  E.  Henry  in  his  monograph  on  TAT 
Analysis  (1).  He  defines  two  areas  of  TAT  content  interpretation: 
positive  content  and  negative  content.  "The  difference  between 
positive  and  negative  content  can  be  summarized  in  this  way:  the 
analysis  of  positive  content  is  concerned  with  what  the  subject  actu- 
ally has  said,  the  analysis  of  negative  content  is  concerned  with  what 
the  subject  has  failed  to  say  and  with  what  he  might  have  been  ex- 
pected to  say  considering  the  usual  responses  made  to  that  picture." 

Normative  and  validating  research  on  the  significant  avoidance 
scores  based  on  these  statistical  characteristics  of  the  tests  for  Levels  I, 
II,  and  III  is  now  being  done.  Since  these  studies  have  not  been  com- 
pleted, the  Level  IV  avoidance  scores  are  not  included  in  this  volume. 

There  are  two  additional  sources  of  Level  IV  significant  avoidance 
data  which  will  now  be  briefly  reviewed.  Both  of  these  are  based  on 
special  psychological  techniques  for  measuring  the  selective  factors  in 
memory  and  perception. 

A  test  of  repression  developed  by  Boris  Iflund  (2)  seems  to  pro- 
vide an  excellent  measurement  of  Level  IV  data.  The  Iflund  test  deals 
with  selective  memory  factors.  It  consists  of  34  cards,  each  of  which 
contains  a  picture.  Twenty-eight  of  these  are  illustrations  of  personal- 
ity needs  (as  listed  by  Murray).  Six  pictures  are  bu^er  cards  which 
contain  nonloaded  (street  or  landscape)  scenes.  The  subject  is  told 
that  he  will  be  shown  the  stack  of  cards,  each  card  being  exposed  for 
5  seconds.  He  is  told  that  after  the  entire  deck  has  been  shown  to  him 
he  will  be  asked  to  recall  as  many  as  he  can.  After  the  subject  has  re- 
called as  many  as  he  can  the  nonbuffer  (i.e.,  thematic)  cards  which  he 
recalled  are  removed  from  the  deck.  The  same  process  of  presenta- 
tion and  recall  is  repeated  until  all  cards  have  been  recalled.  In  indi- 
vidual administration  an  inquiry  period  after  the  test  is  employed  to 
clarify  accuracy  or  misperceptions  of  the  cards. 

This  test  is  based  on  the  psychoanalytic  theory  of  repression.  It  is 
held  that  the  themes  which  the  subject  remembers  last  are  subject  to 
more  repression  than  those  he  recalls  first.  The  serial  order  of  recall 
is  believed  to  relate  to  intrapsychic  defensive  processes  holding  sensi- 
tive themes  for  awareness. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  UNEXPRESSED  199 

To  the  extent  that  the  Iflund  test  does  isolate  warded-ofF  themes, 
it  can  be  used  as  an  estimate  of  the  stratification  of  personality  and  as 
an  indicator  of  Level  IV  themes.  The  most-forgotten  themes,  i.e., 
those  remembered  last,  should  be  close  to  the  Level  IV  omission  score. 

Research  on  the  Iflund  test  is  currently  uncompleted  and  is  not 
included  in  the  systematic  and  diagnostic  studies  reported  in  this  book. 

A  third  technique  for  determining  Level  IV  significant  avoidance 
scores  has  been  suggested.  This  involves  the  theory  of  perceptual 
vigilance  or  perceptual  defense  in  relation  to  sensitive  or  warded-off 
emotional  stimuli.  Methods  have  been  developed  by  experimental 
psychologists  for  determining  the  level  of  perceptual  readiness  to  re- 
spond to  varied  stimuli.  The  tachistoscope  (which  is  a  machine  for 
exposing  stimuli  cards  to  an  observer  at  split-second  speeds)  is  em- 
ployed in  these  experiments.  There  is  some  evidence  suggesting  that 
the  speed  of  recognition  varies  in  relation  to  the  emotional  loading  of 
the  stimuli.  Subjects  whose  personalities  are  mobilized  to  ward  off 
hostility  from  awareness  tend  to  require  slower  speeds  of  presentation 
in  order  to  perceive  hostile  motifs  on  cards. 

To  the  extent  that  this  theory  of  motivated  perception  and  per- 
ceptual defense  holds  true,  tachistoscope  recognition  speed  becomes  an 
estimation  of  Level  IV.  The  themes  which  are  recognized  most 
quickly  should  be  those  which  are  expressed  or  consciously  claimed  at 
the  upper  levels.  Those  which  are  recognized  at  the  slowest  tachisto- 
scope speeds  should  also  be  avoided  at  the  levels  of  action  and  con- 
scious report.  Perceptual  defense  thus  becomes  an  estimate  of  the 
significantly  avoided  and  a  measure  of  Level  IV. 

References 

1.  Henry,  W.  E.  The  Thematic  Apperception  Test  technique  in  the  study  of  culture- 
personahty  relations.  Genet.  Psychol.  Monogr.,  1947,  35,  3-135. 

2.  Iflund,  B.  Selective  recall  of  meaningful  materials  as  related  to  psychoanalytic 
formulations  in  certain  psychiatric  syndromes.  Unpublished  doctor's  dissertation. 
University  of  California,  Berkeley,  1953. 


11 


The  Level  of  Values:  The  Ego  Ideal 


There  Is  another  area  of  human  behavior  which  because  of  its  func- 
tional value  and  theoretical  uniqueness  has  been  designated  as  one  of 
the  operating  levels  of  personality.  This  is  Level  V — the  level  of 
values.  It  includes  the  interpersonal  aspects:  ideals  held  by  the  indi- 
vidual— his  conceptions  of  "lightness,"  "goodness,"  of  w^hat  he  should 
like  to  be. 

The  last  four  chapters  have  presented  Levels  I  through  IV,  working 
sequentially  from  the  public  overt  aspects  of  behavior  into  the  more 
private,  unexpressed  areas.  At  this  point  the  trend  is  reversed.  The 
numerical  designation  of  Level  V  suggests  that  this  is  the  deepest 
level — which,  of  course,  it  is  not.  Level  V  is  concerned  with  con- 
sciously reported  ideals.  The  subject  is  asked  to  list,  or  describe,  or 
check  his  picture  of  how  he  should  like  to  be. 

Level  V  Is  aji  Independent  Area  of  Fersonality 

Level  V,  as  presently  measured  in  the  interpersonal  system,  is  not 
a  very  complicated  or  deep  measurement.  It  simply  gives  us  a  pic- 
ture of  how  the  subject  wants  us  to  see  his  ideals.  It  tells  us  which 
values  he  consciously  stresses.  The  subject  may  privately  have  dif- 
ferent goals  and  stress  different  feehngs.  His  private  value  system 
may  be  in  contradiction  to  his  openly  reported  principles. 

The  working  procedures  of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  proj- 
ect simplify  this  complexity  of  values.  The  general  empirical  ap- 
proach of  this  system  of  personahty  is  to  select  several  narrow  opera- 
tionally defined  areas  of  behavior  (which  are  called  levels)  and  to 
utihze  the  same  matrix  of  interpersonal  variables  to  measure  behavior 
at  these  levels.  The  levels  are  defined  by  the  nature  of  the  data,  that  is, 
by  the  way  it  is  obtained,  by  the  technical  context  of  the  measurement 
process.  If  the  subject  produces  fantasy  themes  in  response  to  pro- 
jective test  stimuli  then  Level  III  is  defined.  When  he  attributes  inter- 
personal themes  to  his  ideal  then  Level  V  is  defined. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  VALUES  201 

Although  Level  V  is  a  rather  simple  measure  of  consciously  re- 
ported values,  it  possesses  a  clear-cut  statistical  independence.  That  is 
to  say,  it  does  not  duplicate  the  other  levels.  Patients'  descriptions  of 
their  ideals  are  very  often  quite  different  from  their  conscious  self- 
descriptions  and  their  fantasy  expressions.  This  measure  seems  to  vary 
independently  of  the  other  levels.  This  offers  reasons  for  expecting 
that  it  may  serve  a  unique  psychological  function  and  possess  a  unique 
clinical  application. 

The  Universality  of  Value  Systems 

The  Level  V  value  system  gives  us  a  picture  of  the  interpersonal 
standards  which  the  subject  holds  for  himself.  The  notion  of  ideal,  it 
should  be  noted,  is  widely  accepted  and  natural.  During  our  testing 
procedures  patients  readily  take  to  the  task  of  describing  their  ideal. 
The  concepts  of  "right"  and  "good"  and  the  interpersonal  themes 
associated  with  these  values  seem  to  be  taken  for  granted. 

The  vital  and  universal  process  of  idealization  has  been  recognized 
by  almost  every  personality  theorist.  Kluckhohn  and  iMurray  ( 1 ,  p.  2 1 ) 
for  example,  state  that:  "One  of  the  important  establishments  of  a  per- 
sonality is  the  ideal  self,  an  integrate  of  images  which  portrays  the 
person  'at  his  future  best,'  realizing  all  his  ambitions."  The  related 
concepts  of  ideal,  superego,  and  introjection  have  been  receiving  in- 
creasing emphasis  in  psychoanalytic  theory. 

Ethical  standards  appear  to  exist  in  all  cultures.  In  an  earlier  chap- 
ter the  universality  of  symboHc  behavior  was  noted.  The  same  can  be 
said  for  moral  standards  of  conduct.  It  is  hard  to  conceive  of  a  society 
or  a  social  group  which  does  not  possess  many  principles  of  "rightness" 
and  "wrongness."  Not  all  the  members  of  a  society  necessarily  share 
(publicly  or  privately)  the  same  set  of  values.  Some  may  assign  posi- 
tive values  to  force,  others  to  deceit,  others  to  charity.  In  many  so- 
cieties women  may  publicly  accept  standards  which  are  different  from 
men's. 

Although  the  specific  qualities  to  which  "goodness"  and  "bad- 
ness" are  attached  may  vary,  what  seems  to  remain  constant  is  the  as- 
sumption that  there  are  standards  of  right  and  wrong. 

It  is  these  principles  which  we  tap  in  Level  V  when  we  study  the 
interpersonal  themes  which  the  subject  idealizes  and  those  which  he 
avoids  attributing  to  his  ideal. 

The  Function  of  Value  Systems 

This  poses  the  questions:  Why  do  individuals  develop  standards 
and  ethical  principles  of  behavior?  What  is  the  function  of  these 
ideakc^ 


202  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

A  detailed  discussion  of  this  topic  is  well  beyond  the  scope  of  this 
book,  encompassing  as  it  does  the  genesis  and  meaning  of  ethics  and 
morals.  The  general  assumption  about  human  motivation  employed 
in  this  book  does  suggest  certain  approaches  to  these  questions. 

The  basic  function  of  the  individual's  interpersonal  behavior  is  to 
ward  off  survival  anxiety.  Any  personality  pattern  can  be  viewed  as 
an  attempt  to  come  to  terms  with  the  social  environment.  In  this  light 
the  development  and  maintenance  of  value  systems  can  be  seen  as  pro- 
viding several  bulwarks  against  anxiety. 

First  of  all,  the  acceptance  of  certain  ideals  tends  to  link  the  indi- 
vidual to  strong  forces  in  his  world.  By  taking  over  and  expressing 
these  ideals  the  subject  identifies  himself  with  powerful  images  of 
rightness.  The  standards  may  come  from  his  parents,  from  his  con- 
ception of  religious  figures,  from  the  standards  held  by  his  social 
groups,  etc.  Generally  they  come  from  all  these  sources.  Symonds 
(2)  has  described  the  process  by  which  the  individual  combats  his 
feelings  of  weakness  and  develops  a  feeling  of  omnipotence  by  taking 
as  models  and  values  those  of  the  group:  "One  looks  for  support  by 
acceding  to  the  wishes  of  society  through  its  laws  and  customs,  so 
that  one  feels  secure  as  a  member  of  the  group  and  derives  power  from 
the  group.  A  still  further  development  is  to  align  oneself  with  the 
universe  and  to  look  to  God  for  strength.  So  the  religious  person,  by 
obeying  the  rules  of  morality,  is  continuing  this  process  of  gaining 
strength  for  himself  by  aligning  himself  with  superior  forces." 

By  taking  on  standards  and  ideals  the  individual  wins  approval  and 
attempts  to  ward  off  disapproval.  Heightened  self-esteem  and  the 
avoidance  of  shame  and  inferiority  can  be  achieved  by  the  acceptance 
and  expression  of  value  systems.  It  appears  that  all  human  beings 
maintain  this  one  unique  area  of  their  personality  which  reflects  their 
conception  of  what  they  should  or  could  be. 

Like  behavior  at  other  levels,  the  value  system  may  play  a  de- 
structive and  unsettling  role  in  the  total  personality  structure.  Ideals 
which  are  too  elevated  or  standards  which  are  too  strict  may  lead  to 
severe  conflict  with  other  levels.  Thus  the  Level  II  self-conception 
may  fall  far  short  of  a  rigid,  demanding  set  of  ideals — with  a  resulting 
feeling  of  guilt  and  self-dissatisfaction.  This  phenomenon  has  been 
noted  at  other  levels  where  extreme,  exaggerated  behavior  at  one  level 
creates  new  circles  of  conflict  and  anxiety. 

Human  beings  presumably  develop  standards  in  an  attempt  to  lessen 
anxiety,  win  approval,  or  to  win  security  through  linkage  with  power- 
ful parental  and  societal  forces.  But  the  complexity  of  social  adjust- 
ment generally  creates  the  tragic  paradox  of  humaa  nature — the  tech- 


THE  LEVEL  OF  VALUES  203 

niques  for  avoiding  anxiety  at  one  level  are  related  to  the  activities  of 
other  levels.  They  cause  tension  at  other  levels  w^hich  in  turn  may 
increase  anxiety  and  lead  to  an  increase  in  the  original  behavior. 

Kluckhohn  and  Murray  ( 1 )  have  commented  on  certain  aspects  of 
this  process.  They  point  out  the  relationships  of  aspirations  and  ideals 
to  the  "frustration  and  dissatisfaction"  of  overt  behavior.  "High  as- 
pirations can  cause  unhappiness  and  discontent,  while  the  process  of 
low^ering  aspirations  to  realizable  levels  is  functional." 

This  process  of  relaxing  standards  is,  however,  not  a  simple  or 
voluntary  procedure.  The  ideals  held  by  individuals,  like  the  behavior 
expressed  at  any  other  level,  are  not  easily  changed.  This  is,  we  pre- 
sume, because  they  play  a  vital  functional  role  in  the  total  personality 
pattern.  Human  beings  develop  ideals  for  the  very  important  purpose 
of  warding  off  survival  anxiety  and  avoiding  shame,  weakness,  and 
disapproval.  These  ideals  develop  and  are  expressed  in  reciprocal 
response  to  the  activities  of  other  levels.  Frustrations  (and  the  ac- 
companying anxiety  produced)  at  Level  I  may  result  in  a  lowering  of 
the  associated  Level  V  ideals;  or  they  may  result  in  an  increase.  Inter- 
personal behavior  at  any  level  of  personality  has  the  function  of  ward- 
ing off  anxiety.  This  behavior  can  shift  in  response  to  stimuli  from  the 
external  environment  or  in  relationship  to  pressures  or  changes  from 
other  levels  of  personality.  A  most  complex  chain  of  multilevel 
processes  is  involved  in  any  interpersonal  pattern  at  any  single  level. 
The  interpersonal  ideals  which  we  measure  at  Level  V  are  not  excep- 
tions to  this  principle.  In  the  latter  sections  of  this  chapter  some  of 
these  multilevel  relationships  (the  indices  of  self-acceptance  and  ideali- 
zation) which  involve  Level  V  and  the  other  establishments  of  the  per- 
sonality will  be  reviewed. 

The  Measurement  of  Interpersonal  Ideals 

To  obtain  measurements  of  Level  V  behavior  it  is  necessary  to  have 
the  subject  communicate  his  system  of  values.  His  ideals  are  then 
categorized  in  terms  of  the  continuum  of  sixteen  interpersonal  var- 
iables. These  scores  are  then  treated  in  the  same  way  as  the  scores  from 
other  levels;  they  can  be  formalized,  standardized,  diagramed,  and 
then  related  to  the  total  multilevel  pattern. 

There  are  many  methods  for  obtaining  the  raw  protocol  data  for 
Level  V.  The  subject  can  be  asked  to  describe  his  ideals  either  in 
interview  or  in  essay  form.  He  can  be  given  check  lists  or  question- 
naires about  his  values.  Regardless  of  how  the  data  are  collected  the 
rating  procedure  is  the  same.  The  interpersonal  aspects  of  these  ex- 
pressions are  coded  into  the  language  of  the  interpersonal  system. 


S04 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


At  the  present  time  the  Kaiser  Foundation  project  is  employing 
three  methods  for  obtaining  Level  V  data. 

Scores  from  the  interpersonal  adjective  check  list  on  which  the  pa- 
tient rates  his  ego  ideal  are  coded  Level  V-C. 

Ratings  by  trained  personnel  of  the  subject's  ideals  as  expressed  in 
diagnostic  interviews  are  coded  Level  V-Di;  in  therapy  interviews, 
Level  V-Ti. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  routinely  obtains  Level  V 
protocols  (along  with  measures  at  seven  other  areas  or  levels)  as  part 
of  the  personality  test  battery.  The  key  measuring  instrument  in  this 
process  is  the  interpersonal  check  list.  Each  patient  uses  this  check 
list  to  rate  first  himself  then  his  parents,  his  spouse,  and  his  ego  ideal. 
The  instructions  for  the  Level  V-C  test  request  the  patient  to  check 
the  items  which  describe  "his  ideal,  his  picture  of  himself  as  he  should 
like  to  be."  The  patient  is  thus  allowed  to  describe  his  value  system  on 
the  same  measuring  instrument  which  he  has  employed  to  describe 
himself  and  three  important  family  members.  The  empirical  ad- 
vantages of  this  procedure  for  comparing  behavior  at  different  levels 
has  been  previously  discussed. 

Figure  25  presents  the  Level  V-C  profile  of  a  patient  tested  in  the 
Kaiser  Foundation  clinic.  For  comparison  we  have  also  included  the 


LEVEL    Il-C   CONSCIOUS 
SELF-DESCRIPTION 


LEVEL   V-C   EGO   IDEAL 


Figure  25.  Illustrations  of  Level  II-C  and  Level  V-C  Profiles  for  a  Docile  Patient 
Whose  Ego  Ideal  Involves  Strength. 


Level  II-C  self  profile.  These  diagrams  indicate  that  the  patient  sees 
himself  as  a  weak,  docile  person.  His  ego  ideal  stresses  themes  of 
strength  and  power. 


THE  LEVEL  OF  VALUES  205 

The  Functional  Value  of  Level  V 

Level  V  is  a  simple  but  useful  diagnostic  tool.  Its  first  and  most 
obvious  application  is  the  insight  it  gives  us  into  the  subject's  value 
system.  Human  beings  vary  in  the  interpersonal  themes  they  idealize. 
Some  stress  congeniality  and  conventional  agreeability.  Others  em- 
phasize strength  and  assertion.  Some  prize  competition.  Others  are 
concerned  with  frank,  blunt  honesty,  or  modest  reserve.  There  seem 
to  be  relationships  between  diagnostic  types  and  the  nature  of  the 
ego  ideal.  Patients  who  stress  dominance  or  submission  at  Level  I-M 
emphasize  pure  power  in  their  ego  ideal.  Patients  who  manifest  either 
hostility  or  love  at  Level  I-M  are  significantly  less  concerned  with 
power  and  more  involved  with  friendliness  in  their  ideals.  In  other 
words,  subjects  whose  actions  fall  along  the  vertical  axis  place  their 
ideals  at  the  top  of  the  vertical  axis  (dominance).  Subjects  whose  ac- 
tions locate  on  the  horizontal  axis  place  their  ideals  close  to  the  hori- 
zontal axis  (affection). 

Another  and  perhaps  the  most  important  use  of  the  Level  V  score 
is  obtained  by  comparing  it  with  other  levels.  The  discrepancy  be- 
tween ideal  and  Level  II  Self  provides  an  index  of  self -acceptance. 
This  variable  plays  a  most  crucial  role  in  arousing  motivation  for 
therapy.  Similarly,  the  kind  and  amount  of  discrepancy  between  the 
ideal  and  the  conscious  descriptions  of  family  members  provides  an- 
other set  of  valuable  indices.  These  discrepancies  are  called  the  in- 
dices of  idealization. 

The  essence  of  these  and  the  other  interlevel  discrepancies  between 
the  ego  ideal  and  the  other  areas  of  personality  is  as  follows:  once  we 
systematically  locate  the  subject's  ego  ideal  we  can  compare  all  the 
measures  from  the  other  levels  of  personality  to  see  how  close  they 
fall  to  the  ego  ideal.  Different  theoretical  and  clinical  implications 
are  attached  to  these  idealization  indices. 

Limitations  of  the  Level  V  Score 

The  measurement  of  ego  ideal  employing  the  interpersonal  adjec- 
tive check  list  is  somewhat  limited  because  of  the  tendency  of  all  pa- 
tients to  stereotype  their  ideals.  In  one  sample  of  207  routine  clinic 
intake  patients,  53  per  cent  placed  their  ego  ideal  in  the  managerial 
octant  and  37  per  cent  in  the  responsible-hypernormal  octant.  This 
means  that  90  per  cent  of  all  patients  had  ego  ideals  In  the  upper  right- 
hand  quadrant  and  less  than  2  per  cent  placed  their  ego  ideal  in  the 
lower  (weak)  half  of  the  diagnostic  circle. 

This  homogeneity  is  a  cultural  stereotype.  We  might  expect  that 
some  other  cultures  would  stress  aggression  and  some  (oriental,  for 


2o6  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

example)  might  idealize  the  passivity  and  modesty  which  American 
urban  subjects  so  dramatically  devaluate. 

References 

1.  Kluckhohn,  C,  and  H.  AIurray  (eds.).  Personality  in  nature,  society  and  culture. 
New  York:  Alfred  A.  Knopf,  Inc.,  1949. 

2.  Symonds,  p.  The  dynamics  of  human  adjustment.  New  York:  Appleton-Century- 
Crofts,  Inc.,  1946. 


12 


A  System  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis' 


We  have  suggested  in  Chapter  6  that  functional  diagnosis  of  personal- 
ity involves  two  basic  dimensions — interpersonal  behavior  and  varia- 
bility. The  preceding  five  chapters  of  this  section  have  dealt  with  the 
concepts  and  measurement  techniques  by  which  we  systematize  the 
interpersonal  dimension  of  personality.  We  have  defined  the  inter- 
personal variables  by  which  security  operations  of  the  human  being 
can  be  classified.  We  have  considered  the  levels  at  which  this  behavior 
is  observed. 

With  these  theories  and  methods  as  background,  it  is  now  possible 
to  consider  the  application  of  this  personality  system  for  interpersonal 
diagnosis. 

Purpose  of  Personality  Diagnosis 

An  examination  of  psychiatric  nosology  reveals  considerable  varia- 
tion in  terms.  Some  diagnostic  categories  refer  to  the  symptomatic 
picture.  Some  are  clearly  moralistic  or  evaluative  epithets,  e.g.,  in- 
adequate personality.  Some  refer  to  character  traits,  e.g.,  obsessive- 
compulsive.  Some  are  global  terms  for  disease  entities  which  sum- 
marize many  specific  factors,  e.g.,  schizophrenic  and  hysteric. 

In  order  to  evaluate  these  terms  it  is  necessary  to  inquire  into  the 
purpose  of  personahty  diagnosis.  Just  why  do  we  need  a  nosology? 
Just  what  is  it  to  be  used  for? 

Psychiatric  or  personality  diagnosis  terms  actually  have  many  dif- 
fering uses,  depending  on  the  institutional  or  cultural  context.  In  the 

*  Appendix  4  presents  an  inrerpersonal  diagnostic  report  written  for  the  psychiatric 
clinic.  This  report  serves  as  a  clinical  illustration  of  the  theory  and  methodology  pre- 
sented in  this  chapter.  The  execution  of  multilevel  interpersonal  diagnosis  is  facili- 
tated considerably  by  the  use  of  a  printed  booklet  in  which  the  tabular  and  diagram- 
matic steps  involved  in  diagnosis  are  organized.  This  "Record  Booklet  for  Inter- 
personal Diagnosis  of  Personality"  also  includes  a  simplified  procedure  for  calculating 
interlevel  discrepancies  (variabihty  indices),  for  plotting  them  on  a  profile  sheet,  and 
for  preparing  verbal  summaries  of  the  indices.  A  copy  of  this  booklet  is  presented  in 
Appendix  4. 

207 


2o8  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

legal  situation,  to  take  an  extreme  example,  the  diagnostician  is  usu- 
ally called  upon  to  determine  whether  the  patient  is  grossly  psychotic 
or  not.  The  judicial  authorities  are  generally  not  the  least  bit  interested 
in  the  fine  shadings  of  ego  organization  or  the  complexities  of  the 
oedipal  situation.  A  single  "yes"  or  "no"  as  to  the  sanity  of  the  sub- 
ject is  generally  sufficient. 

Many  psychiatric  centers  employ  broad  categories  which  are  just 
as  gross  and  dichotomous.  Some  admitting  wards,  observation  cen- 
ters, etc.,  have  the  sole  mission  of  deciding  where  the  patient  will  be 
routed.  If  the  patient  is  markedly  psychotic,  he  goes  to  a  psychiatric 
hospital;  if  he  is  neurotic,  he  is  sent  back  to  the  community.  For  such 
purposes,  these  agencies  do  not  require  subtle  differential,  diagnostic 
categories. 

In  most  psychiatric  hospitals  the  diagnostic  decisions  are  somewhat 
more  complex.  Differential  diagnosis  relates  to  differential  treatment 
plans.  One  type  of  patient  may  be  assigned  to  electric  shock  therapy, 
another  to  insulin  treatment.  Therapies  of  this  sort  are  aimed  at 
symptom  removal  and  not  character  reorganization.  The  diagnostic 
and  prognostic  terms,  therefore,  tend  to  focus  on  the  descriptive  or 
symptomatic  aspects  of  behavior.  An  interpersonal  or  characterologi- 
cal  diagnostic  system  is,  in  this  case,  not  the  most  relevant  tool.  In  de- 
ciding between  two  types  of  physical  therapies  the  nature  of  the  pa- 
tient's interpersonal  reflexes,  the  type  of  repressed  motivation  or  the 
expected  transference  are  not  the  central  criteria. 

Descriptive  diagnosis  as  presented  in  psychiatric  textbooks  is  gen- 
erally considered  to  be  crude,  unreliable,  and  nontheoretical  (1,  2,  3, 
5).  The  main  reason  for  the  slow  progress  in  psychiatric  nosology 
parallels,  perhaps,  the  general  crudeness  of  most  psychotherapeutic 
techniques.  Medical  diagnosis  is,  by  comparison,  extremely  specific 
and  definitive  because  of  the  differentiated  maturity  of  medical  knowl- 
edge. There  are  hundreds  of  detailed  medical  diagnoses  all  pointing 
to  specific  medical  treatment  plans.  By  contrast,  psychological  theory 
and  psychiatric  practice  is  most  limited.  This  situation  seems,  how- 
ever, to  be  improving. 

R.  E.  Harris,  for  example,  points  out:  "With  progress  in  psycho- 
therapy, diagnosis  is  becoming  more  and  more  a  matter  of  assaying 
'therapeutically  relevant'  variables,  i.e.,  those  which  are  related  to 
what  happens  in  therapeutic  interviews — the  resistances,  the  defenses, 
the  strength  of  the  ego,  the  amount  and  quality  of  the  anxiety  and  its 
sources,  the  quality  of  reality  testing,  etc.  These  variables  are  defined 
and  understood  both  as  they  are  inferred  from  the  historical  recon- 
struction of  the  life  history  and,  more  importantly,  as  they  appear  in 
the  interaction  between  therapist  and  patient."   (4,  pp.  27-28) 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


209 


The  modem  American  psychiatric  clinic  is  taking  on  an  increasing 
number  of  prognostic  decisions.  A  survey  of  the  intake  procedures 
of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychiatric  Clinic  revealed  that  there  are 
over  twenty  ways  of  disposing  of  a  case.  Most  of  these  decisions  are 
based  on  characterological  or  interpersonal  factors:  the  amount  of  ego- 
alien  anxiety,  the  interpersonal  techniques  for  handling  anxiety,  the 
kind  and  amount  of  motivation  for  personality  exploration  and  change. 
Certain  "pure"  hysterics  and  psychosomatic  patients  whose  bland,  ego- 
syntonic,  hypernormal  adjustments  mobilize  against  psychotherapy 
might  be  sent  back  to  the  referring  physician  with  recommendations 
for  supportive  medical  handling.  Other  hysteric  or  psychosomatic 
patients  whose  personality  patterns  reveal  underlying  feelings  of  de- 
pression, anxiety,  or  deprivation  might  be  referred  to  specific,  care- 
fully delimited  therapeutic  relationships.  Patients  are  assigned  to  sev- 
eral kinds  of  individual  psychotherapy  depending  on  the  nature  of 
the  personality  picture.  Three  types  of  group  therapy  are  available 
for  certain  kinds  of  patients. 

In  a  psychiatric  clinic  of  this  sort  interpersonal  predictions  which 
define  the  amount  of  anxiety  and  the  interpersonal  patterns  to  which 
it  is  attached  are  most  useful.  For  patients  beginning  the  long  road  of 
orthodox  psychoanalysis,  interpersonal  diagnosis  is  less  relevant. 
Transference  factors  are  provoked  and  dealt  with  during  the  lengthy 
process,  and  preanalytic  predictions  might  not  necessarily  save  any 
time  or  energy.  In  the  clinic,  however,  where  flexibility  of  treatment 
program  is  emphasized,  predictions  as  to  expected  behavior,  expected 
resistances,  and  cues  concerning  the  nature  of  repressed  motives  are 
at  a  premium.  Interpersonal  variables  which  measure  the  social  re- 
activity of  the  patient,  overt  and  covert,  current  and  future,  appear 
to  have  the  highest  functional  "cash  value." 

From  the  standpoint  of  practical  application  and  research  objectiv- 
ity, interpersonal  diagnostic  patterns  seem  superior  to  Kraepelinian  or 
psychiatric  diagnostic  terms.  This  is  not  to  say  that  standard  psychi- 
atric nomenclature  should  be  or  could  be  abandoned.  There  are,  in- 
deed, several  factors  which  argue  against  the  offhand  rejection  of 
psychiatric  language.  In  a  following  section  we  shall  seek  to  discover 
relationships  and  communalities  between  interpersonal  and  psychiatric 
diagnoses.  Such  relationships,  if  they  exist,  would  greatly  broaden  the 
functional  and  theoretical  power  of  both  diagnostic  systems. 

Any  personality  diagnosis,  thus,  serves  several  ends.  It  provides  a 
classification  most  useful  for  administrative,  legal,  predictive,  and 
research  purposes.  A  more  detailed  interpersonal  diagnosis  serves  the 
added  function  of  predicting  the  kind  and  sequence  of  security  opera- 
tions to  be  expected  from  the  patient. 


2IO  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

In  the  following  pages  we  shall  present  a  diagnostic  system  com- 
prising 65,536  categories.  These  are  based  on  the  permutations  of 
interpersonal  measurements  "adjustive  and  maladjustive"  at  three  lev- 
els (four  layers)  of  behavior.  This  is  an  automatic  diagnostic  pro- 
cedure. It  is  based  on  the  logic,  theory,  and  methodology  which  have 
been  discussed  in  the  preceding  chapters.  There  is  no  clinical  judg- 
ment or  psychological  intuition  involved  in  making  an  interpersonal 
diagnosis.  The  personality  data  are  collected  and  fed  into  the  nota- 
tional  apparatus,  and  the  diagnosis  automatically  rolls  out.  In  the 
Kaiser  Foundation  Clinic  the  tests  of  Level  I  and  II  are  scored  by 
clerical  workers;  the  themes  of  Level  III  are  rated  by  nonprofes- 
sional technicians.  The  resulting  indices  are  plotted  on  standardized 
graphs,  and  a  diagnosis  involving  three  levels  of  interpersonal  be- 
havior, as  well  as  a  diagnosis  of  variability,  is  obtained.  Not  one  minute 
of  professional  time  is  required  for  these  systematic  diagnoses.  (An 
independent  clinical  diagnosis  is,  of  course,  prepared  by  the  psychiatric 
intake  worker,  and  the  two  assessments  of  the  patient  are  fitted  to- 
gether in  the  clinical  evaluation  conference.) 

Systematic  diagnosis  based  on  multilevel  test  batteries  is  much  more 
precise  and  detailed  than  clinical  diagnosis.  It  is  quite  difficult  to  ob- 
tain reliable  diagnoses  when  clinicians  use  only  their  own  percep- 
tions and  observations.  Studies  of  the  reliability  of  psychiatric  judg- 
ments are  notoriously  low  even  when  the  simplest,  broadest  fourfold 
categories  are  employed  (1,  2,  3,  5).  If  clinicians  were  asked  to  select 
a  diagnosis  from  a  list  of  65,536  categories,  an  endlessly  long  period  of 
intensified  training  would  be  necessary  to  effect  any  kind  of  reliability. 

The  systematic  diagnoses  we  are  about  to  consider  require,  how- 
ever, no  intuitive  decision.  The  categorization  system  works  some- 
thing like  a  table  of  logarithms  or  square  roots.  Much  time  and  some 
creativity  have  gone  into  the  development  of  these  mathematical  tables 
— but  to  use  them  is  a  routine,  clerical,  and  reliable  procedure.  What- 
ever clinical  experience  and  theoretical  competence  was  available 
has  been  built  into  the  classification  schema.  The  application  of  the 
system  is  a  rote  process.  The  professional  worker  then  takes  the  re- 
sults of  the  systematic  operations  and  fits  them  to  his  clinical  knowl- 
edge of  the  case. 

Three  Systematic  Methods  for  Summarizing  Personality 

The  meaning  and  function  of  diagnosis  will  be  made  clearer  if  we 
compare  it  with  the  other  methods  of  summarizing  personality  data. 
The  Kaiser  project  has  employed  three  different  kinds  of  assessments 
of  human  personality — each  with  its  own  purpose  and  significance  and 
each  possessing  certain  limitations  and  advantages.    These  are:    (1) 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  2 1 1 

the  diagnostic  code-formula  of  personality,  (2)  the  diagnostic  profile 
of  personality,  and  (3)  the  diagnostic  description  of  personality. 

The  diagnostic  code  is  a  succinct,  systematic  multilevel  label  or 
coded  formula  which  is  taken  from  a  finite  standardized  list  of  mutu- 
ally exclusive  terms.  These  terms  should  be  operationally  defined. 
The  diagnostic  classification  should,  therefore,  be  highly  reliable.  Its 
primary  purpose  is  to  summarize  the  essence  of  the  multilevel  pattern 
of  personality,  to  make  predictions  with  known  probability  about  cer- 
tain crucially  important  aspects  of  behavior,  and  to  prognosticate  the 
success  of  specific  clinical,  therapeutic  techniques. 

The  multilevel  diagnostic  code  can  be  calculated  by  well-trained 
clerical  workers.  The  predictions  can  also  be  produced  by  clerical 
procedures  since  they  involve  the  looking  up  of  probability  indices 
based  on  group  statistics.  A  highly  skilled  clerical  worker  with  no 
knowledge  of  psychological  theory  could  hypothetically  assemble  the 
raw  data,  feed  them  into  the  tables  and  indices,  and  arrive  at  the  auto- 
matic multilevel  code  diagnosis.  This  worker  could  then  enter  a  set 
of  correlation  matrices  and  make  probability  statements  about  the 
patient.  Statements  of  the  following  nature  could  be  made:  'This  pa- 
tient is  an  overtly  autocratic  personality;  less  than  20  per  cent  of  pa- 
tients with  this  Level  I  diagnosis  enter  individual  psychotherapy;  of 
those  patients  who  do  enter  individual  psychotherapy,  more  than  65 
per  cent  quit  within  six  weeks;  of  those  who  enter  group  therapy, 
more  than  40  per  cent  quit  within  six  weeks;  etc." 

The  advantages  of  the  diagnostic  code  include  brevity,  reliability, 
finite  listing  of  possibilities,  and  the  fact  that  it  does  not  require  the 
expenditure  of  professional  energy.  The  disadvantages  are  numerous: 
it  has  no  explanatory  value;  it  predicts  only  for  a  group  (i.e.,  in 
probability  figures)  and  takes  no  account  of  the  unique  complexity  of 
each  individual. 

The  diagnostic  profile  of  personality  presents  a  diagrammatic  and 
numerical  summary  of  the  patient's  behavior  at  each  level,  and  of  his 
variability  indices.  It  summarizes  his  behavior  in  terms  of  the  inter- 
personal measurements,  and  in  terms  of  the  operationally  defined 
indices  of  personality  organization.  The  procedures  on  which  the 
profile  is  based  have  a  known  reliability. 

The  diagnostic  profiles  can  be  prepared  by  well-trained  technicians, 
i.e.,  semiprofessional  workers  with  a  rudimentary  knowledge  of  per- 
sonality theory.  These  technicians,  in  the  Kaiser  project,  administer 
the  testing  batteries.  They  supervise  the  scoring  of  the  Level  I  and 
II  tests.  They  work  in  teams  to  score  the  fantasy  material  in  terms  of 
the  interpersonal  variables.  They  perform  the  necessary  arithmetic 
procedures — calculating  the  horizontal  and  vertical  indices,  convert- 


212  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

ing  them  to  standard  scores.  They  plot  the  interpersonal  scores  on 
the  diagnostic  grids.  They  measure  the  discrepancies  among  these 
scores  and  thus  determine  the  amount  of  the  variability  indices.  They 
plot  the  variability  profile. 

With  these  two  diagnostic  patterns — the  variability  and  interper- 
sonal profiles — it  is  possible  to  classify  patients  in  a  most  detailed  man- 
ner. Eight  interpersonal  indices  (at  four  levels)  and  14-18  variability 
indices  are  available  for  automatic  interpretation.  These  technicians 
are  capable  of  translating  this  matrix  of  scores  into  statements  which 
summarize  the  personality.  They  can  report  the  behavior  at  Levels 
I,  II,  III,  and  V.  They  can  indicate  the  amount  of  each  variability 
mdex.  A  considerable  mass  of  finely  graduated  information  can  be 
routinely  reported  for  clinical  or  research  purposes.  In  addition  to 
these  detailed  classificatory  statements,  the  personahty  profiles  al- 
low for  a  battery  of  individual  predictions.  Each  of  the  eight  inter- 
personal and  the  many  variability  indices  have  a  set  of  empirical  facts 
related  to  them.  The  Level  II  versus  V  discrepancy  (self-description 
versus  ego  ideal)  if  high  defines  low  self-acceptance  or  high  motiva- 
tion for  treatment.  This  is  correlated  highly  with  entering  and  remain- 
ing in  psychotherapy.  This  variability  index,  thus,  leads  to  a  specific 
clinical  prediction.  The  other  indices  have  similar  prognostic  applica- 
tions. 

The  personality  profile  serves,  in  this  way,  as  a  precise,  detailed 
classificatory  system,  and  as  a  source  for  numerous  specific  clinical  pre- 
dictions. It  has,  however,  limited  explanatory  value.  It  is  also  re- 
stricted because  of  its  routine  objective  nature.  The  personality  pro- 
files are  rehable — they  do  not  involve  speculation  or  intuition.  The 
variability  indices  are  ground  out  automatically,  but  they  fail  to  inte- 
grate the  complex  network  of  scores.  They  do  not  in  any  way  lessen 
the  necessity  for  clinical,  professional  interpretation.  They  rather 
serve  as  a  highly  articulated  assistance  to  the  clinician. 

The  personality  diagnostic  report.^  In  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Clinic 
the  professional  diagnostician  enters  the  picture  after  the  personality 
profiles  have  been  plotted.  All  the  testing,  scoring,  tabulating,  and 
statistical  predictive  procedures  are  accomplished  by  technical  work- 
ers. The  task  of  the  professional  clinicians  is  to  weave  the  multi- 
dimensional pattern  of  scores  and  probabiUty  statements  into  a  unique, 
meaningful  summary  which  fits  a  particular  patient.  This  is  ac- 
complished by  means  of  the  personality  diagnostic  report. 

The  main  task  of  the  diagnostic  classifications  is  to  categorize  re- 
liably. The  profiles  provide  a  long  list  of  standardized  probability 

*  A  sample  diagnostic  report  written  about  an  illustrative  patient  is  contained  in 

Appendix  4. 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  2 1 3 

statements.  The  diagnostic  report  has  the  function  of  explaining  the 
personality.  The  clinician  studies  all  aspects  of  the  interpersonal  pro- 
file and  relates  them  to  the  case. 

The  intake  worker's  notes  on  family  history  are  compared  with 
the  patient's  view  of  mother,  father,  and  spouse — and  with  his  fantasy 
images.  The  indices  of  motivation  and  prognosis  are  compared  with 
the  patient's  symptomatic  presentation.  All  the  available  clinical  data 
are  reviewed  in  light  of  the  personality  profiles  and  indices.  In  this 
way,  the  experience  of  the  clinician  is  brought  to  bear  on  the  evalua- 
tion process.  The  thousandfold,  multifaceted  pattern  of  the  patient's 
situation  can  be  assembled  in  the  mind  of  the  professional  worker. 

The  diagnostic  evaluation  and  the  predictions  are,  of  course,  in- 
creased in  efficiency  as  the  clinical  material  qualifies  and  amplifies 
the  conclusions  of  the  more  routinized  profiles.  The  diagnostic  report 
allows  room  for  the  creativity  and  insight  which  no  systematic  schema 
can  duplicate.  The  profiles  are,  of  course,  analogous  to  the  laboratory 
and  radiological  indices  provided  to  the  medical  diagnostician.  The 
final  diagnosis  and  prescription  is  based  on  the  multiple  correlation 
procedure  of  great  complexity  which  takes  place  in  the  mind  of  the 
professional  worker. 

The  diagnostic  report  deals  with  such  a  vast  array  of  cues — clini- 
cal and  systematic — that  it  is  highly  individualized.  It  is  less  reliable 
than  the  classification  and  profile  ratings.  It  has  a  margin  of  unrelia- 
bility which  must  be  hazarded  because  of  the  complexity  of  the  sub- 
ject matter. 

From  the  research  standpoint,  the  personality  report  possesses  a 
great  value.  The  correlations  and  results  by  which  we  test  yesterday's 
hypotheses  are  provided  by  the  objective  indices  of  the  interpersonal 
and  variability  profiles.  In  our  scientific  validation  procedures  we  do 
not  rely  on  the  intuitive  personality  report  for  proof.  The  qualitative 
clinical  report  has,  however,  an  inestimable  research  value  because  it 
produces  the  hypotheses  of  tomorrow.  Scientific  progress  in  personal- 
ity psychology  works  upward  from  the  clinical,  creative  speculations 
which  are  first  expressed  in  the  personality  reports.  The  profiles,  it  will 
be  noted,  serve  admirably  to  give  objective  tests  to  hypotheses  and  to 
yield  probability  predictions.  Their  very  objectivity,  however,  guar- 
antees that  they  will  never  generate  a  new  idea. 

The  future  of  personality  research  lies  in  the  front  lines  of  the 
functional  situation.  The  neat  predictions  from  the  research  office  get 
dented  and  pushed  around  by  the  rough  pressure  of  human  individual- 
ity. We  know  very  well  that  the  indices  do  not  take  into  account  the 
familial,  occupational,  cultural,  or  educational  history  of  the  patients 
for  whom  they  attempt  to  predict.   At  exactly  the  spots  where  the 


214 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


z  d 
<  o 


(So        <>J 

15  t  5J  a 


bo  c^ 
■>  c  -  ... 
n    u    V 


w    "^    C    S 


O    3 
"C    O 


S  5  .- 
><  i-i  n 


S      1^ 

5  t!  =  2   a. 
U  u  C  a. " 


c  o  o  "  2  «J 
T3  -D  c  </)  E  Cc 
S  S  ^:2  E  2 
OC^'-B  So  «  CL 


.2^  p  ^-a  i  § 

•£   c  o   u  u,  .5 

b  o  8  =3  I  g  2 

a'lb-iis.s 


2  ^u 
i  E  3 

<  S  y 


S-5 


«    O    O    4J  i2    C  2 

n  60     ^  u  2  y 

u  2  c  2  Ji  ul  3 

Q- rt  o  <u  S-y  S 


c  w       — 
W  J^  S  il 


o  Ch' o  -a  ' 

-o  u  E  "  : 

S  a-O  b' 


c  c  ."H  x: 


o 

w    <0    C  -o 

i'E  «^:2 


>  rt  S  i;  n 


X  p-2 


is  E 


S  2  I 


QtCo: 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  215 

predictive  indices  fail,  the  clinician  is  present  to  observe  and  to  inte- 
grate. If  the  predictions  hit  an  accuracy  of  90  per  cent  they  still  fail  on 
ten  patients  in  every  hundred.  Each  ten  failures  may  be  related  to  new 
clinical  variables  which  may  later  be  added  to  the  system,  recast  in 
operational  language,  and  tested  for  validity.  Inaccurate  predictions 
are  not  embarrassments  but  sources  of  new  hypotheses. 

All  three  elements  of  diagnosis — the  classification,  the  profiles,  and 
the  report — are  necessary  for  optimal  evaluation  of  personality.  Each 
has  its  limitations — each  makes  its  unique  contribution  to  the  clinical 
and  research  aspects  of  the  science.  Many  problems  and  frictions  in 
psychology  might  be  lessened  if  the  nature  and  function  of  these  three 
elements  is  kept  straight.  Table  6  presents  a  summary  of  these  three 
types  of  diagnostic  procedures  which  are  now  employed  in  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  project. 

Functional  Diagnosis 

The  evaluation  of  personality  which  we  are  describing  in  this  book 
is  called  functional  diagnosis.  The  aim  of  our  measurements  is  to  un- 
derstand the  patient-in-his-relationship-to-the-clinic  and  to  make  pre- 
dictions about  the  patient-in-relationship-to-his-future-therapist.  The 
focus  of  our  diagnostic  observations  is  the  interpersonal  behavior  in 
the  context  in  which  we  (the  clinical  staff)  have  commerce  with  him. 
The  results  of  a  psychological  test  battery  can  be  studied  from  many 
vantage  points.  It  is  possible  for  psychologists  to  predict  competence 
in  combat  flying,  academic  success  in  college,  occupational  fitness,  etc. 
These  predictions  may,  in  certain  situations  (in  the  air  force,  on  the 
campus,  in  the  factory) ,  be  quite  functional  and  relevant.  They  would 
not  be  much  help  to  the  psychiatrist  attempting  to  decide  the  best  plan 
of  psychotherapy. 

Psychologists  often  use  clinical  tests  to  estimate  the  patient's 
creativity,  constriction,  impulsivity,  etc.  These  findings  have  some 
value  in  the  understanding  of  personality,  but  they  would  be  of  little 
use  to  the  clinician  who  wants  to  know:  "Why  is  this  patient  coming 
to  the  clinic?  How  much  and  what  kind  of  motivation  is  present? 
How  will  he  react  to  different  types  of  treatment? " 

Other  psychologists  are  able  to  outline  with  impressive  sophistica- 
tion the  probable  genetic  history  of  the  patient,  the  early  traumatic 
events,  and  the  finely  detailed  nuances  of  the  patient's  sexual  adjust- 
ment. For  patients  who  enter  psychoanalysis  or  long-term  treatment 
these  predictions  undoubtedly  point  ahead  to  materials  which  will 
emerge  in  the  associative  content.  The  Kaiser  Foundation  project  has 
not  attempted  to  measure  these  areas  of  personality — partly  because 
they  are  beyond  the  scope  of  our  technical  capacity,  and  partly  be- 


2i6  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

cause  we  believe  them  to  be  less  functional  in  regard  to  the  crucial 
aspects  of  pretherapy  planning  and  of  therapeutic  interaction. 

The  first  aim  of  functional  diagnosis  is  to  summarize  before  treat- 
ment the  aspects  of  the  personality  which  have  a  bearing  on  the  choice 
of  treatment.  What  is  the  motivation  of  the  patient  in  coming  to  the 
clinic?  Does  he  come  with  self-depreciation,  ready  to  unburden  his 
innermost  thoughts  and  expecting  some  kind  of  mystical  cure  to  fol- 
low his  confidences?  Disappointment  and  bitter  reproach  may  be  the 
easily  predicted  outcome  if  this  motivation  is  not  perceived  and 
planned  for.  Does  he  come  under  pressure  from  someone  else  (e.g.,  a 
physician),  defensively  mobilized  against  any  self-examination?  A 
stubborn  power  struggle  and  angry  departure  may  be  predicted  if  this 
motivation  is  not  recognized  and  responded  to. 

Clinical  diagnosis  concerns  ego  factors  which  influence  the  choice 
of  treatment.  How  much  anxiety  is  manifested?  What  are  the  se- 
curity operations  by  which  the  patient  handles  anxiety?  What  is  the 
interpersonal  pressure  put  by  the  patient  on  the  chnic? 

The  first  aim  of  functional  diagnosis  is,  then,  to  assess  motivation  for 
treatment.  The  second  aim  is  prognosis  of  treatment — to  summarize 
the  kind  of  behavior  which  will  appear  in  future  therapy.  How  fast 
or  slow  will  be  the  course  of  therapy?  Many  patients  who  are  well 
motivated  for  change  (thus  satisfying  the  first  criterion  of  functional 
diagnosis)  also  manifest  chronic,  deeply  rooted  security  operations 
which  are  most  resistant  to  change,  or  underlying  psychotic  distrust 
which  had  best  be  left  unexplored. 

Another  aspect  of  prognosis  concerns  the  nature  of  the  intrapsychic 
conflicts.  In  many  cases  it  is  possible  to  point  to  private  or  "precon- 
scious"  motives  which  will  probably  afl^ect  the  later  treatment  rela- 
tionship. A  different  transference  relationship  and  prognosis  are  indi- 
cated depending  on  whether  the  underlying  themes  involve  distrustful, 
passive  resistance,  or  independent  autonomy,  or  nurturant  tenderness. 

In  making  our  prediction  about  the  first  (or  motivational)  aspect  of 
functional  diagnosis  we  are  mainly  interested  in  "ego"  factors.  In 
making  the  second  prediction  we  concentrate  on  the  deeper,  "pre- 
conscious"  aspects  of  personality  and  their  relationship  to  the  more 
overt  or  public  factors.  We  use  the  total  interpersonal  profile  to  map 
out  areas  of  anxiety,  the  security  operations  by  which  it  is  handled,  and 
the  transference  phenomenon  which  they  will  tend  to  elicit  during 
psychotherapy. 

Interpersonal  Typology 

To  accomplish  functional  diagnosis  the  total  personality  pattern 
(both  the  interpersonal  and  variability  profiles)   is  employed.    In- 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  217 

volved  in  this  total  matrix  are  thousands  of  individual  measurements 
which  are  summarized  in  terms  of  sixteen  interpersonal  diagnostic 
circles  and  an  extensive  assortment  of  variability  indices.  The  com- 
plexity of  this  system  of  diagnosis  is  such  that  it  is  almost  impossible 
that  any  two  patients  would  ever  show  exactly  the  same  kind  of  multi- 
level profile. 

For  this  reason  a  typological  system  is  needed.  jMethods  have  been 
developed  for  summary  classification  of  interpersonal  and  variability 
types.  In  this  chapter  we  shall  consider  the  interpersonal  typology.^ 

Two  Kinds  of  Interpersonal  Typologies.  We  recall  that  inter- 
personal behavior  has  been  rated  at  five  levels.  Two  of  these — Level  I 
communication  and  Level  II  self-description — are  considered  to  refer 
to  the  overt  picture  which  the  patient  expresses  in  the  clinical  situa- 
tion. These  are  designated  presenting  operations.  A  technique  has 
been  worked  out  for  typing  or  categorizing  the  more  overt,  conscious 
aspects  of  the  patient's  personality.  We  shall  speak,  for  example,  of 
the  managerial  type  or  the  over  conventional  personality. 

The  categories  which  serve  to  summarize  these  behavioral  opera- 
tions do  not  directly  apply  to  the  underlying  motives  of  the  patient. 
The  "preconscious"  themes  of  Level  III  tend  to  require  a  different 
typological  language.  In  an  earlier  chapter  we  have  noted  that  sub- 
jects in  their  fantasy  themes  express  motives  which  are  more  intense 
and  extreme  than  those  which  they  manifest  in  their  overt  behavior  or 
conscious  self-descriptions.  For  this  reason  it  is  necessary  to  develop 
a  typology  for  summarizing  the  underlying  operations  of  the  patient. 
The  presenting  operations  (of  Levels  I  and  II)  are,  therefore,  sum- 
marized in  terms  of  behavioral  types,  and  the  underlying  operations 
are  categorized  in  terms  of  thematic  motives.  The  specific  diagnostic 
classes  of  these  two  aspects  of  personality  will  now  be  presented. 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Presenting  Operations.  The  first 
goal  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  is  to  summarize  the  overt  behavioral 
impact  of  the  patient  upon  the  clinic.  The  task  here  is  to  determine: 
(1)  the  kind  of  security  operations,  (2)  the  adaptive  or  maladaptive 
pattern  of  conflict.  This  is  accomplished  as  follows.  First,  the  Level 
II  Self  and  the  Level  I  symptomatic  scores  are  converted  into  hori- 
zontal and  vertical  indices  and  plotted  on  the  diagnostic  grid.'' 

These  summary  placements,  it  will  be  noted,  reflect  two  aspects  of 
the  Level  I  and  II  behaviors.  The  sector  of  the  circle  tells  us  what 
interpersonal  operations  are  involved,  and  the  distance  from  the  center 

^The  variability  dimension  of  personality  is  discussed  briefly  in  Chapter  13. 

"  The  methodology  for  converting  conscious  self-descriptions  and  "symptomatic 
communications"  into  horizontal  and  vertical  indices  and  for  plotting  these  indices  on 
the  diagnostic  grid  has  been  explained  in  earlier  chapters. 


2l8 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


tells  us  how  extreme  or  intense  they  are.  Figure  26  presents  an  illus- 
trative diagraming  of  the  Level  I-M  and  II-C  scores  for  an  overcon- 
ventional  patient.  Both  the  Level  I-M  and  II-C  scores  fall  in  the  M 
sector  of  the  circle,  and  both  fall  in  the  outer  ring  of  the  circle,  indi- 
cating that  they  are  more  than  one  sigma  above  the  mean  in  the  direc- 


.^''' 
.^' 


MANAGERMt- 


Up; 

Auroc/f 


ATic 


^<r. 


/ 

\s^^      /                   \ 

/■. 

^__^.^ 

-v 

/     \,  \ 

/  \^ 

•I 

° ,,, 1 ,,,,?,,, , 

]0 

'0 

60 

•» 

\      /. 

\X'' 

E» 

I'l-F^ 


^JF^ 


2^[^;«AS0CH1STIC 


Figure  26.  The  Summary  Placements  of  the  Level  I-M  and  II-C  Scores  for  an 
Overconventional  Patient.  Key:  The  center  of  the  grid  js  determined  by  the  intersec- 
tion of  the  means  of  the  honzontal  and  vertical  distributions.  The  grid  is  calibrated  in 
standard  score  units. 

tion  of  conventionality  and  blandness.  Two  typological  procedures 
allow  us  to  classify  this  pattern.  The  first  step  is  to  summarize  the 
sixteen-variable  matrix  into  eight  diagnostic  categories.  This  is  ac- 
complished by  combining  adjacent  variables  and  assigning  a  descrip- 
tive term  which  reflects  the  interpersonal  meaning.  Pairing  the  sixteen 
variables  yields  eight  sectors  of  the  diagnostic  circle,  which  are  called 
octants.  Thus  we  combine  A  (forceful  dominance)  with  ?  (respected 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


219 


success)  into  a  power  octant,  and  we  combine  B  (self-confident  inde- 
pendence) with  C  (competitive  self-seeking)  into  a  narcissistic  octant. 
We  also  express  in  our  diagnostic  summaries  the  adjustive  or  mal- 
adjustive  aspects  of  the  presenting  operations.  If  the  subject's  behavior 
falls  in  the  outer  ring  of  the  circle  (one  sigma  above  the  mean),  an 
intense  or  maladaptive  degree  of  this  interpersonal  behavior  is  indi- 
cated If  his  security  operations  fall  within  one  sigma  of  the  center  of 
the  circle,  a  moderate,  adaptive  degree  is  indicated.  A  statistical  tech- 
nique for  diagnosing  normality-abnormality  or  adaption-maladaption 
in  terms  of  degree  is  thus  available.   Figure  27  presents  a  schematic 


AP 


BC 


NARCISSISTIC 
PERSONALITY 


AUTOCRATIC 
PERSONALITY 


NO 


DE 


.  p  COMPETITIVE 
^     PERSONALITY 


SADISTIC         / 
PERSONALITY    / 


MANAGERIAL   /Av         HYPERNORMAL 
personality/^  ^v^     PERSONALITY 

\ 
\ 

RESPONSIBLE    \ 
PERSONALITY     ^ 


AGGRESSIVE 
PERSONALITY 


COOPERATIVE     I 
PERSONALITY  qvER- 


^  REBELLIOUS 
\  PERSONALITY 
\ 

"^v  /      SELF- 

DISTRUSTFUL    ^,    ^/     EFFACING 
PERSONALITY        ^V    PERSONALITY 


'    CONVENTIONAL/ 
/      PERSONALITY  /  Liyi 


DOCILE  \    / 

PERSONALITY^^ 

/ 

/ 


FG 


MASOCHISTIC 
PERSONALITY 


DEPENDENT 
PERSONALITY 


HI 


JK 


Figure  27.  The  Diagnosis  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  at  Levek  I  and  II. 


illustration  of  this  method  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  and  illustrates  the 
fact  that  there  are  eight  interpersonal  diagnostic  categories  by  which 
we  summarize  the  overt  behavioral  presentation  of  the  patient  and  that 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


there  is  an  adaptive  and  a  maladaptive  degree  to  each  type.  These  eight 
adjustive  and  maladjustive  categories  are  formally  listed  in  Table  7. 


TABLE  7 
The  Adaptive  and  Maladaptive  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Types 


The  Adfiistive  Inter- 
personal Types  {one 
sigma  or  less  from 
Interpersonal  the  mean)  and  the  Nu- 

Variable  merical  Code  Used  to 

Code  Designate  Them. 

AP  1  Managerial  personality 

BC  2  Competitive  personality 

DE  3  Aggressive  personality 

FG  4  Rebellious  personality 

HI  5  Self-effacing  personality 

JK  6  Docile  personality 

LM  7  Cooperative  personality 

NO  8  Responsible  personality 


The  Maladjustive  Inter- 
personal Types  {one  sigma 
above  the  mean)  and  the 
Numerical  Codes  Used  to 
Designate  Them. 

Autocratic  personality 
Narcissistic   personality 
Sadistic  personality 
Distrustful  personality 
Masochistic  personality 
Dependent  personality 
Overconventional  personality 
Hypernormal   personality 


It  v^^ill  be  noted  that  for  each  adaptive  and  maladaptive  type  there 
is  a  verbal  descriptive  category  (e.g.,  conventional;  overconven- 
tional) and  a  numerical  code.  The  numerical  designation  for  the  con- 
ventional type  is  7,  and  for  the  overconventional  type  it  is  7.  The 
numerical  index  is  a  most  convenient  w^ay  of  summarizing  behavior  at 
any  level  because  the  digits  can  be  combined  into  diagnostic  formulas. 
An  italic  diagnostic  digit  always  refers  to  the  moderate  amount  of  the 
interpersonal  trait.  A  roman-face  diagnostic  digit  always  refers  to  the 
intense  or  extreme  amount  of  the  behavior.  This  system  of  numerical 
code  diagnosis  will  be  discussed  in  later  sections  of  this  chapter. 

These  diagnostic  terms  are  employed  to  summarize  security  opera- 
tions at  Levels  I  and  IL  The  diagnosis  is  accomplished  automatically 
by  locating  the  indices  for  Level  I  in  the  correct  octant  sector  and  se- 
lecting the  interpersonal  term  which  reflects  this  particular  behavior. 
This  yields  the  Level  I  diagnosis.  The  same  procedure  is  followed  for 
Level  II — the  indices  determine  the  proper  sector,  and  the  appropriate 
term  is  thus  determined. 

A  printed  booklet  for  deriving  a  multilevel  interpersonal  diagnosis 
is  presented  in  Appendix  4. 

Figure  28  presents  an  illustration  of  Level  I  and  Level  II  diagnosis 
for  two  sample  patients. 

The  Level  I-M  scores  for  patient  "X"  fall  within  one  sigma  of  the 
mean  of  the  NO  octant.  Referring  to  Table  7  we  see  that  this  defines 
an  adaptive  degree  of  responsible  behavior.  Patient  "X"  is  therefore 
diagnosed  at  Level  \-\{  as  a  Responsible  Personality  (numerical  code 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  221 

diagnosis  =  8).  The  Level  II-C  score  for  this  patient  is  in  the  same 
octant  but  beyond  the  normal  range.  This  patient  is  self-diagnosed  as 
a  Hypernormal  Personality  (numerical  code  diagnosis  =8).  For  rou- 
tine diagnostic  research  categorization  of  what  we  have  called  "pre- 
senting operations,"  the  Level  I  and  II  diagnostic  terms  can  be  com- 
bined in  a  hyphenated  designation,  which  for  patient  "X"  would  be 
Responsible-Hypernormal  Personality.  A  more  convenient  summary 
method  is  to  combine  the  numerical  codes,  which  for  patient  "X" 
would  be  8S.  The  first  term  (verbal  or  numerical)  in  any  diagnostic 
formulation  always  refers  to  Level  I-M  symptomatic  behavior,  and 
the  second  term  to  Level  II-C  self-description. 

The  Level  I-M  of  the  other  illustrative  patient  "Y"  falls  in  the  outer 
ring  of  the  HI  sector.  This  (by  reference  to  Table  7)  is  seen  to  define 
a  Masochistic  Personality  at  Level  I-M  (numerical  code  diagnosis 
=  5).  The  Level  II-C  scores  fall  in  the  extreme  end  of  the  JK  octant. 
The  Level  II-C  diagnosis  is:  Dependent  Personality  (numerical  code 
diagnosis  =  6).  The  combined  diagnosis  of  presenting  operations  is 
Masochistic-Dependent  personality,  or  more  simply  a  ''56"  personality 
type. 

In  practice,  the  single  level  diagnosis  is  rarely  employed.  The  focus 
of  the  functional  diagnosis  generally  includes  the  combined  presenting 
operations  of  Level  I-M  plus  Level  II-C.  There  are  sixteen  possible 
categories  for  diagnosis  at  Level  I-M — eight  adjustive  and  eight  mal- 
adjustive  types.  The  same  number  of  categories  are  available  for  sum- 
marizing Level  II-C  behavior  when  we  turn  to  the  combined  diag- 
nosis involved  in  the  presenting  operations.  There  are,  therefore,  256 
categories  for  interpersonal  diagnosis  of  presenting,  or  facade 
operations. 

To  present  a  diagnostic  system  involving  256  types  would  seem  to 
be  an  audacious  gesture,  placing  an  impossible  task  on  the  diagnostician 
who  attempts  to  use  this  system.  It  has  already  been  pointed  out  that 
diagnosis  in  the  interpersonal  system  is  a  most  routine  and  unde- 
manding procedure.  What  we  ask  of  a  diagnostic  label  is  an  objective 
summary  categorization  for  administrative  or  research  classificatory 
purposes.  We  do  not  ask  our  diagnosis  to  take  the  place  of  a  personal- 
ity profile  or  a  personality  description.  It  is,  instead,  an  automatic  and 
rehable  classification.  In  practice,  the  interpersonal  diagnosis  is  rou- 
tinely determined  by  clerical  procedures.  The  patient's  scores  on 
Levels  I  and  II  are  placed  on  the  diagnostic  grid,  and  the  appropriate 
diagnostic  terms  or  two-digit  diagnostic  codes  are  automatically  de- 
termined. The  theory  and  methodology  of  the  interpersonal  system 
is  complex,  but  the  employment  of  the  system  for  the  purpose  of  diag- 
nostic classification  is  simple. 


222 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


Figure  28.  Illustration  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  at  Levels  I-M  and  II-C  for  Two 
Patients,  "X"  and  "Y"  {see  facing  page) . 


Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  "Underlying  Operations."  The  sec- 
ond goal  of  functional  diagnosis  is,  we  recall,  to  summarize  the  the- 
matic "preconscious"  motifs,  to  relate  them  to  the  overt  presenting  op- 
erations, and  to  employ  these  data  to  make  predictions  about  the  fu- 
ture course  of  treatment.  We  shall  now  consider  a  method  for  sum- 
marizing the  "preconscious"  themes  of  Level  III. 

The  theory  and  measurement  methods  for  Level  III  fantasy  expres- 
sions were  presented  in  Chapter  9.  It  was  emphasized  that  there  are 
two  layers  of  "preconscious"  data — the  themes  assigned  to  fantasy 
heroes  and  those  assigned  to  fantasy  "others."  These  sublevels  involve 
different  psychological  functions.  They  have  different  lawful  rela- 
tionships with  the  other  aspects  of  personality  structure.   These  two 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


223 


K^^^^' 


MANAGffiMt 


^^ 


,N^ 


,>^V 


Uj 


'^uroc, 


'%/, 


'J>>: 


.^0 


/A 


•I 


ffr. 


^is 


•n 


^2^>(S0CHIST\C 

^(HiJ ■ 


^^5^^ 


PATIENT      Y 

Figure  28  (cont.) 

sublevels  provide  two   diagnostic  types  of  underlying  operations. 

In  Chapter  9  we  presented  a  method  of  measuring  the  interpersonal 
themes  from  fantasy  data  and  for  converting  the  resulting  scores  into 
indices  which  locate  the  subject  on  the  diagnostic  grids  for  Level  III-T 
Hero  and  Level  III-T  Other.  There  remains  the  task  of  formally  di- 
viding the  Level  III-T  grids  into  summary  diagnostic  sectors.  This  is 
accomplished  in  the  same  manner  as  for  Levels  I-M  and  II-C.  The 
sixteen-variable  matrix  is  combined  into  the  same  eight  sectors  and  the 
appropriate  descriptive  terms  or  numerical  codes  assigned.  The  Level 
III  diagnostic  circle  is  presented  in  Figure  29. 

In  comparing  the  diagnostic  grid  for  Level  III  with  that  used  for 
Levels  I  and  II,  two  differences  will  be  observed.  The  first  concerns 
terminology — Level  III  deals  with  underlying  private  motives  and 
not  behavioral  manifestations.  The  diagnostic  terms  tend,  therefore, 
to  reflect  general  motivational  purposes  rather  than  social  role  be- 


2  24 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


2      EXPLOITATION 


SADISM 


DEPRIVATION 


POWER 


NURTURANCE 


/^\ 


LOVE 


DEPENDENCE 


MASOCHISM 


Figure  29.  The  Diagnosis  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  at  Level  III  Hero  and  Other. 

havior.  A  second  difference  concerns  the  degree  of  intensity.  At 
Levels  I  and  II  we  distinguish  between  adaptive  behavior  and  mal- 
adaptive extremes.  At  Level  III  no  such  distinction  is  maintained  in 
setting  up  verbal  descriptive  categories.  There  are  two  reasons:  Theo- 
retically it  is  questionable  that  "preconscious"  imagery  can  be  con- 
ceptualized as  being  adaptive  or  maladaptive.  In  one  sense  it  appears 
that  this  differentiation  violates  the  notion  of  the  equilibrium  or 
"safety  valve"  function  of  fantasy.  In  some  cases  the  most  violent 
and  antisocial  autism  may  serve  a  healthy  balancing  function.  The  sec- 
ond reason  for  not  making  the  adaptive-maladaptive  distinction  at 
Level  III  tends  to  make  the  first  argument  academic.  At  this  point  our 
measures  of  Level  III  fantasy  are  so  crude  and  preliminary  that  the 
fine  distinctions  of  normal  versus  abnormal  autisms  have  been  difficult 
to  study.    The  distinction  between  italicized  numerical  digits   (for 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  225 

moderate  behavior)  and  roman-face  digits  (for  intense  behavior)  is 
maintained  at  Level  III,  but  it  simply  reflects  the  amount  of  the  inter- 
personal emotion. 

There  are,  then,  eight  verbal  summary  categories  and  sixteen  nu- 
merical categories  for  diagnosing  behavior  at  Level  III  Hero  and  Other. 
We  have  been  continually  reminded  by  one  of  the  basic  mottoes  of 
this  book  that  no  level  has  meaning  by  itself,  and  each  level  must  be 
interpreted  in  light  of  the  other  levels.  Some  theories  of  personality 
do  tend  to  diagnose  on  the  basis  of  underlying  motives.  The  Kaiser 
Foundation  system,  on  the  contrary,  does  not  follow  this  procedure. 
We  may  tend  to  overemphasize  the  presenting  operations  (i.e.,  diag- 
nosing from  Levels  I  and  II),  but  complete  diagnosis  includes  the  un- 
derlying thematic  behavior. 

Multilevel  Interpersonal  Diagnosis.  We  are  now  ready  to 
proceed  to  the  complete  interpersonal  diagnosis  of  the  two  sample 
cases  whose  behavior  we  have  been  considering  in  this  chapter.  Inter- 
personal diagnosis  is  accomplished  by  combining  the  sumiTmry  descrip- 
tive terms  for  Self  behavior  at  Levels  I  and  II,  and  the  Self  and  Other 
behavior  at  Level  III.  The  significant  omissions  of  Level  IV,  and  the 
behaviors  of  ''others''  at  all  levels  (except  III),  and  the  value-themes  of 
Level  V  are  not  included  in  the  diagnostic  categorization.  It  must  also 
be  kept  in  mind  that  we  are  dealing,  in  this  chapter,  with  the  inter- 
personal aspects  of  diagnosis.  The  variability  diagnosis,  which  com- 
prises the  other  half  of  functional  diagnosis,  will  be  treated  in  Chap- 
ter 13. 

Figure  30  presents  the  Self  scores  of  two  patients,  "X"  and  "Y,"  at 
the  three  levels  (I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T)  which  comprise  interpersonal 
diagnosis.  The  methodology  for  plotting  the  summary  scores  on  the 
diagnostic  grid  has  already  been  described.  The  diagnostic  classifica- 
tion is  automatically  obtained  from  Table  7  (for  the  Level  I-M  and 
II-C  terms)  and  Figure  30  (for  the  Level  III  terms).  The  Level  I-M 
score  for  patient  "X"  falls  in  the  adaptive  ring  of  the  NO  octant  (nu- 
merical code  =  S),  his  Level  II-C  score  in  the  maladaptive  range  of  the 
same  octant  (numerical  code  =  8),  the  Level  III-T  Hero  and  Other 
scores  in  the  DE  octant  (both  coded  3).  His  interpersonal  diagnosis 
is:  responsible-hypernormal  personality  nvith  underlying  hostile  feel- 
ings. The  four  digit  diagnostic  code  is  5*833. 

The  Level  I-M  and  II-C  scores  for  Patient  "Y"  fall  in  the  mal- 
adaptive ring  of  sectors  FG  and  JK  respectively  (coded  46).  The 
"preconscious"  Hero  scores  are  in  the  outer  ring  of  the  BC  octant 
(coded  2)  and  the  "preconscious"  Other  scores  are  in  the  inner  ring 
of  the  HI  octant  (coded  J).   Referring  to  Table  7  (for  the  Level  I 


226 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


^;^^* 


U^p; 


.#' 


^/§/  m-T 

Q 


/::\^. 


"-^W 


10c 


5^A 


^^^. 


1^2^S0CHIST\C 


X 


n-c 


/ 

\ 

\^           ^  T 

r ""  ■/ 

\ 

/  \ 

^^^^^-^—-^ 

nr-o 

,0 

2\l^ 

'/h^ 

|.      M?M, 

,    ,   ,     '|°     ,   M    1   ,  ,    ,    ,' 

^__-^ 

_j^^ 

\\i 

L-^^^ 

^/ 

[\  /^\ 

/ 

Figure  30.  Illustration  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C  and  III-T 
for  Two  Patients  ("X"  and  "Y").  Key:  For  Levels  I-M  and  II-C  the  circular  dotted 
line  indicates  the  distance  of  one  standard  deviation  from  the  mean.  (The  mean  for 
both  the  honzontal  and  vertical  distributions  in  the  center  of  the  circle.)  At  Levels 
1-M  and  II-C  scores  falling  vi'ithin  the  inner  circle  are  considered  adaptive  and  those 
in  the  outer  circle  maladaptive.  At  Level  III-T  the  verbal  distinction  between  adaptive- 
maladaptive  is  not  maintained.  Figure  29  presents  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  cate- 
gories for  Level  III. 

and  II  terms)  and  Figure  29  (for  the  Level  III  terms),  we  see  that  his 
interpersonal  diagnosis  is:  distrustful-dependent  personality  with  un- 
derlying narcissism  and  deeper  feelings  of  masochism.  The  diagnostic 
code  for  this  four-layer  pattern  is  5625. 

The  verbal  diagnostic  formula  comprises  a  four-part  sequence.  The 
Level  I  diagnosis  comes  first,  and  it  is  always  paired  with  the  Level  II 
diagnosis.  The  hyphenated  description  summarizes  the  presenting  op- 
erations of  the  patient.  The  Level  III  Hero  and  Other  diagnoses  fol- 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


227 


^^f' 


MANAGfRMt. 


I-M 


m-H 


A 


'I'^A 


n-c 


•''%^^ 


/ 

-^ 

y 

\ 

\v       /            \ 

/ 

^ '\ 

>0 

,  .  ,    2,0, ,  ,' 

/ 

1  , ,  ,  ,'m  .  , , 

,  ,  ,  ,•<» ,« 

---^^C/ 

\ 

\ 

\ /n-{ 

\ 

-'\ 

/ 

^ThJ) 


.€ 


^vv^"^ 


PATIENT      Y 

Figure  30  (cont.) 


low.   The  standard  grammatical  structure  of  an  interpersonal  diag- 
nosis can  now  be  considered: 

" - Personality  with  Underlying , 


AND  Deeper  Feelings  of 


The  first  two  elements, 


personality,"  are 
The  third  element 
,"  is  obtained 


determined  by  the  Level  I  and  II  summary  scores 

in  the  diagnostic  formula,  "with  underlying 

from  the  Level  III  Hero  summary  scores.  The  fourth  element  comes 
from  the  Level  III  Other  indices.  To  complete  the  functional  diag- 
nosis we  add  the  variability  diagnosis. 

The  Numerical  Diagnostic  Code.  The  numerical  diagnostic 
summary  is  a  four-digit  formula  which  sequentially  presents  the  Level 
I,  II,  III  Hero,  and  III  Other  scores. 


22$  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

This  four-layer  system  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  provides  an  ob- 
jective, reliable,  and  standardized  classification.  The  diagnostic  for- 
mula summarizes  the  four  aspects  of  the  patient's  security  operations 
which  are  most  central  to  the  decisions  which  clinicians  make  about 
patients:  How  does  he  act?  How  does  he  see  himself?  What  are  his 
underlying  interpersonal  potentials? 

A  detailed  and  complex  diagnosis  is  involved  in  the  four-layer 
formula.  It  was  pointed  out  that  there  are  256  types  of  presenting  op- 
erations. Each  of  these  256  diagnostic  types  can  be  characterized  by 
any  one  of  eight  Level  III  Hero  patterns  and  by  any  one  of  eight  Level 
III  Other  patterns.  For  example,  the  patient  whose  presenting  opera- 
tions are  diagnosed  "responsible-hypernormal"  can  have  underlying 
"preconscious"  themes  located  in  any  of  the  eight  Level  III  Hero 
sectors  and  in  any  one  of  the  eight  Level  III  Other  sectors.  There  are 
64  combinations  of  Level  III  Hero  and  Other  scores.  There  are,  there- 
fore, 16,384  (256  X  64)  verbal  diagnostic  formulas  available  for 
summarizing  human  security  operations.  When  we  consider  the  four- 
layer  combination  of  numerical  codes,  a  much  larger  set  of  possibili- 
ties exists.  It  will  be  recalled  that  the  distinction  between  moderate 
and  extreme  behaviors  is  preserved  in  numerical  diagnosis,  italic  num- 
bers referring  to  the  former  and  roman-face  numbers  to  the  latter. 
There  are,  therefore,  65,536  (256X16X16)  numerical  formulas 
available  for  summarizing  interpersonal  behavior  at  four  layers  of 
personality. 

Of  the  16,384  verbal  diagnostic  categories,  one  quarter  characterize 
the  adaptive  or  adjusted  personality  (at  the  level  of  presenting  opera- 
tions). One  quarter  of  them  involve  maladaptive  types.  One  half  of 
them  designate  personalities  who  have  inconsistent  ratings  of  adjust- 
ment-maladjustment. Of  this  latter  group  it  is  obvious,  by  definition, 
that  half  of  them  are  self-diagnosed  as  adaptive  in  the  context  of  a 
symptomatic  (Level  I)  diagnosis  of  maladjustment,  while  the  other 
half  of  this  mixed  group  are  self-diagnosed  as  maladjusted  in  the 
context  of  a  clinical  symptomatic  rating  of  adjustment. 

Each  of  these  four  broad  categories  has  an  obvious  clinical  and 
theoretical  meaning.  A  diagnostic  formula  is,  of  course,  a  crude  and 
rough  estimate  of  the  security  operations  of  the  individuals,  and  it  in 
no  way  can  be  substituted  for  the  more  detailed  pattern  obtained  from 
the  interpersonal  and  variability  profiles  or  from  the  personality  report. 
With  this  qualification  in  mind,  it  can  be  seen  that  a  specific  multilevel 
diagnosis  (of  the  sort  we  have  just  presented)  provides  a  useful  core  of 
relevant  information.  In  one  standardized  four-digit  formula  we  ob- 
tain a  summary  of  (1)  the  interpersonal  behavior  at  three  levels  and 
(2)  an  estimate  of  kind  and  degree  of  adjustment-maladjustment. 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  229 

The  Relationship  of  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  to 
Pyschiatric  (Kraepelinian)  Diagnosis 

In  an  earlier  chapter  it  seemed  pertinent  to  comment  on  the  prob- 
lems created  by  the  novelty  of  the  interpersonal  system.  When  the 
reader  who  has  struggled  to  acquaint  himself  with  sixteen  variables, 
five  levels,  fourteen  variability  indices  is  now  presented  with  16,384 
verbal  diagnostic  types  or  65,536  diagnostic  codes,  the  proliferation 
of  new  terms  and  concepts  may  seem  to  be  getting  out  of  hand.  At 
this  point  it  may  prove  encouraging  to  point  out  that  the  interpersonal 
diagnostic  system  is  not  completely  divorced  from  the  traditional 
language  of  the  clinic.  In  our  diagnostic  formulations  we  employ  a 
behavioristic  and  interpersonal  language  to  summarize  the  patient's  per- 
sonality. We  are  often  called  upon  to  communicate  our  diagnoses  to 
other  clinicians  who  are  not  familiar  with  the  systematic  language  of 
the  interpersonal  system. 

The  interpersonal  language — masochistic,  autocratic,  etc. — has 
the  advantage  of  a  narrow  and  parochial  usage.  The  Kraepelinians' 
diagnostic  language,  by  comparison,  possesses  an  almost  universal 
popularity  of  usage,  but  is  often  vague  and  unclear.  Most  every 
clinician  tends  to  use  labels  such  as  schizoid  or  hysteric,  but  many  of 
them  mean  quite  different  things  by  these  terms.  One  clinician  may 
conceive  of  the  hysteric  in  terms  of  certain  colorful  symptoms;  an- 
other may  refer  to  the  state  of  psychosexual  development;  another 
may  denote  a  certain  pattern  of  repressive  defenses  when  he  employs 
the  term. 

However  obvious  its  drawbacks,  the  Kraepelinian  nosology  has  two 
irrefutable  claims  to  survival — its  widespread  acceptance  and  its  im- 
plicit connotations.  A  great  deal  of  wisdom  has  accumulated  in  the 
folklore  of  psychiatry.  By  this  we  mean  that  a  psychiatric  label 
(e.g.,  hysteric)  is  a  crude,  disorganized  synthesis  of  many  variables 
of  behavior.  Some  of  these  are  inconsistent,  some  subjective  to  the 
user,  some  tautological,  some  unimportant,  some  valuable.  Further, 
it  seems  safe  to  say  that  most  psychiatric  labels  have  some  interpersonal 
factor  loading.  Schizoids  show  different  interpersonal  behavior  from 
hysterics,  or  from  phobics,  or  from  obsessives.  It  follows,  therefore, 
that  there  must  be  considerable  overlap  between  the  standard  Krae- 
pelinian nosology  and  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  system  presented 
in  this  chapter. 

With  this  hypothesis  in  mind  an  investigation  of  the  interpersonal 
factors  in  psychiatric  terminology  was  undertaken  by  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  research  project.  If  this  hypothesis  is  true,  then  several 
advantages  will  accrue  to  both  diagnostic  systems.   The  objectivity. 


j,o  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

reliability,  and  systematic  complexity  of  the  interpersonal  schema  can 
be  related  to  the  standard  Kraepelinian  terminology.  The  latter  might 
be  partially  defined  in  terms  of  the  operational  language  of  the  for- 
mer. The  pathological,  maladjustive  emphasis  of  the  Kraepelinian  sys- 
tem may  be  amplified  by  the  adaptive  dimensions  of  the  interpersonal 
schema. 

Testing  the  Relationship  Between  Interpersonal 
and  Psychiatric  Diagnosis 

The  question  now  posed  might  be  worded  as  follows:  What  is  the 
relation  between  interpersonal  and  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis?  The 
preceding  sections  of  this  chapter  have  presented  a  method  for 
establishing  interpersonal  diagnosis  at  three  levels  (four  sublevels)  of 
personality.  For  comparison  with  Kraepelinian  categories  it  seemed 
logical  to  employ  the  levels  which  define  presenting  operations,  i.e., 
Levels  I  and  II.  For  the  exploratory  investigations  it  was  decided  to 
use  the  Level  II  self-description  as  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  cri- 
terion. The  obtaining  of  an  index  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  was,  thus, 
a  single  straightforward  task.  The  next  problem  was  to  find  a 
measurement  or  rating  of  Kraepelinian  diagnosis  to  compare  with  the 
interpersonal  criterion.  This  was  not  as  easy  a  procedure.  The  most 
obvious  solution  was  to  ask  psychiatric  clinicians  to  make  diagnostic 
judgments  of  the  same  patients  where  Level  II  profiles  were  employed 
for  interpersonal  diagnosis.  This  proved  to  be  unfeasible  for  two 
reasons.  In  the  first  place,  psychiatric  diagnostic  judgments  are  no- 
toriously unreliable  (1,  2,  3,  5).  If  one  side  of  the  comparison  is  an 
undependable  measure,  the  extent  of  the  true  relationship  between 
the  variables  is  clouded.  A  second  disadvantage  of  clinical  diagnostic 
ratings  concerns  the  subjective  factors  unique  to  each  clinician.  Ex- 
tended conversations  with  practicing  clinicians  revealed  a  wide  varia- 
tion in  individual  preferences  for  use  or  avoidance  of  certain  Krae- 
pelinian terms.  Some  psychiatrists  expressed  doubt  as  to  their  ability 
to  employ  certain  diagnostic  categories  satisfactorily.  Others  believed 
that  pure  Kraepelinian  types  were  rare,  and  that  most  patients  show  a 
mixture  of  reactions.  Most  all  of  them  expressed  a  preference  for  dy- 
namic or  psychoanalytic  language. 

The  search  for  a  criterion  measure  of  psychiatric  diagnosis  which 
would  be  reliable  and  standardized  led  us  in  the  direction  of  psycho- 
metric estimates.  The  Minnesota  Multiphasic  Personality  Inventory 
seemed  to  be  a  most  satisfactory  estimate  of  psychiatric  diagnosis  be- 
cause it  is  reliable  and  because  there  are  widely  accepted  patterns  of 
scores  which  are  valid  estimates  of  psychiatric  diagnosis. 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


231 


The  comparison  procedure  can  be  briefly  summarized  as  follows. 
The  MMPI  diagnosis  of  200  clinic  patients  was  determined  (by  pooled 
ratings  of  three  psychologists) . 

Six  of  the  most  common  neurotic  types  or  character  disorders  were 
employed  as  the  diagnostic  criterion.  The  Level  II  diagnoses  of  these 
same  patients  were  obtained.  The  results  are  summarized  in  Table  8. 
They  indicate  that  these  six  psychiatric  diagnostic  types  are  related  to 
different  interpersonal  modes  of  behavior, 

TABLE  8 
Median   Interpersonal  Self-Description  Score  for   Six   MMPI   Clinical   Groups 


Median 

Score 

on  Level 

Verbal 

MMPI 

Number 

II  Inter- 

Summary  of 

Psychiatric 

Sorting 

of 

personal 

Level  II 

Diagnosis 

Criteria 

Cases 

Continuum 

Score 

Psychopathic 

F,  Pd,  Ma 

12 

D 

Aggressive,  sadistic 

Schizoid 

D,  F,  Sc,  Pd 

32 

F 

Bitter,  distrustful 

Obsessive 

D,  Pt 

42 

H 

Self-derogatory,  passive 

Phobic 

D,  Pt,  Hy 

48 

K 

Docile,  dependent 

Hysteric 

Hy,  K 

31 

M 

Bland,  overconventional 

Psychosomatic 

Hs,  Hy 

35 

M-N 

Responsible,   hyper- 
normal,  generous 

Patients  who  employ  aggressive,  nonconventional  modes  of  mal- 
adjustment tend  to  obtain  the  psychiatric  diagnosis  of  psychopathic 
personality.  Distrustful,  passively-resistant  modes  of  adjustment  tend 
to  be  called  schizoid;  submissive,  self-punishing  patients  tend  to  be 
called  obsessives;  docile,  dependent  patients  tend  to  be  labeled  phobics; 
bland,  naive,  overconforming  patients  tend  to  be  diagnosed  hysterics; 
and  responsible,  hypernormal  patients  fit  the  psychosomatic  pattern 
of  the  MMPI. 

Of  the  eight  interpersonal  modes  of  adjustment-maladjustment,  six 
are  related  to  psychiatric  categories.  Two  interpersonal  modes,  how- 
ever, the  autocratic-managerial  and  the  competitive-narcissistic,  seem 
to  have  no  psychiatric  equivalent.  A  new  question  arises:  Why  do 
two  interpersonal  modes  fail  to  merit  psychiatric  diagnostic  cate- 
gories? Why  do  managerial  and  competitive  people  fail  to  excite  diag- 
nostic attention,  and  thus  avoid  formal  psychiatric  recognition?  These 
categories  are  not  unmentioned  in  the  clinical  literature.  Exploitive, 
narcissistic,  power-oriented  techniques  have  been  described  by  Fromm, 
Horney,  and  Sullivan.  Prior  to  these  culturally  oriented  writers  little 
reference  has  been  made  to  these  cases.  They  have  not  obtained 
nosological  popularity  in  any  formal  diagnostic  system. 


232 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 


We  are  led  to  speculate  that  these  types  have  received  little  diag- 
nostic attention  because  they  do  not  come  for  help.  Perhaps  they  do 
not  seek  therapeutic  assistance  because  the  very  essence  of  these  mal- 
adjustments is  a  compulsive  maintenance  of  autonomy,  independence, 
and  domination.  These  social  techniques  clearly  preclude  the  role  of 
a  psychiatric  patient. 

Our  personality  theories  have  generally  been  grounded  in  clinical 
practice.  But  there  seems  to  be  increasing  evidence  that  major  neu- 
rotic groups  exist  which  are  exposed  to  psychological  testing  diagnosis 
and  therapy  in  disproportionately  small  numbers. 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis.  The  data  and  speculations  just  pre- 
sented have  encouraged  the  possibility  of  relating  interpersonal  and 
psychiatric  diagnoses.  It  now  seems  feasible  and  profitable  to  define 
psychiatric  diagnoses  in  terms  of  the  interpersonal  expressions  of  the 
patient.  In  this  manner  we  preserve  the  values  inherent  in  psychiatric 
diagnosis,  its  widespread  acceptance,  and  its  statistical,  administrative, 
and  theoretical  advantages,  which  would  be  lost  by  a  total  rejection  of 
classical  terminology.  For  these  reasons  the  Kaiser  Foundation  re- 
search project  has  retained  the  older  clinical  categories,  combining 
them  with,  and  defining  them  in  terms  of,  interpersonal  factors. 

Table  9  presents  the  eight  modes  of  interpersonal  adjustment  and 
maladjustment  and  the  suggested  psychiatric  categories  to  which  they 
may  be  hnked.  Under  each  interpersonal  category  we  have  listed  not 
one  trait,  but  a  syndrome  of  behaviors  which  are  most  typical,  and 
which  often  seem  to  go  together.  The  category  HI  actually  includes 
many  normal  responses — retiring  modesty,  thoughtful  reserve,  sensi- 
tive, deferent  self-appraisal,  etc.  The  same  HI  sector  of  the  circle 
also  includes  an  assortment  of  extreme,  maladjustive  reactions — pas- 
sive withdrawal,  ruminative  immobilization,  submission,  and  self- 
punitive  attitudes.  A  variety  of  psychiatric  terms  seems  to  be  related 
to  this  generic  interpersonal  mode.  Patients  who  fall  in  this  area  of  the 
diagnostic  circle  are  often  clinically  labeled  masochistic,  guilt-ridden, 
obsessive,  or  psychasthenic.  This  cluster  of  psychiatric  terms  is  thus 
related  to  the  interpersonal  mode.  Wherever  syndromes  of  psychiatric 
categories  or  parallel  diagnostic  terms  exist,  they  are  included  in 
Table  9. 

The  relationships  presented  in  Table  9  mark  an  important  step  in 
the  segmental  development  of  this  book.  They  establish  an  important 
linkage  between  systematic  interpersonal  language  and  standard  clini- 
cal terminology.  They  relate  the  standard  categories  of  psychopath- 
ology  to  a  continuum  of  maladjustive  and  (theoretically  niore  im- 
portant) adjustment  types. 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


233 


TABLE  9 


Operational  Redefinition  of  Psychiatric  Categories  in 
Terms  of  Interpersonal  Operations 


Numer- 
ical       Variable 
Code*       Code 


Interpersonal  Mode 
of  Adjustment 


Interpersonal  Type 
of  Maladjustment 


AP 


BC 


DE 


FG 


HI 


JK 


LM 


Executive,   forceful.    Managing,  auto- 
respected  personality   cratic,  power-ori- 
ented personality 


Independent,  com- 
petitive   personality 

Blunt,  frank,  criti- 
cal, unconventional 
personality 

Realistic,  skeptical 
personality 

Modest,  sensitive 
persona'lity 

Respectful,  trust- 
ful personality 


Narcissistic,   exploit- 
ive personality 

Aggressive,  sadistic 
personality 

Passively  resistant, 
bitter,  distrustful 
personality 

Passive,  submissive, 
self-punishing,  maso- 
chistic  personality 

Docile,  dependent 
personality 


Standard  Psychiatric 
Equivalent  of  Inter- 
personal   Type    of 
Maladjustment 

No  psychiatric 
equivalent   (Com- 
pulsive personality') 

No  psychiatric 
equivalent  (Counter- 
phobic'   Manic') 

Psychopathic,    sadis- 
tic personality 

Schizoid  personality 


Masochistic,  psychas- 
thenic, obsessive 
personality 

Neurasthenic,  mixed 
neurosis,  anxiety 
neurosis,  anxiety 
hysteria,  phobic 
personality 

Hysterical 
personality 


Bland,  conventional.    Naive,  sweet,  over 
friendly,  agreeable       conventional  per- 
personality  sonality 

8  NO        Popular,  responsible    Hypernormal,  Psychosomatic 

personality  hyperpopular,  com-    personality 

pulsively    generous 
personality 

*  The  numerical  codes  for  interpersonal  diagnosis  also  designate  adaptive  or  mal- 
adaptive intensity.  Numbers  in  roman  face  refer  to  extreme  maladjusted  interpersonal 
behavior  and  italicized  numbers  denote  an  adjustive  mode. 

We  define  "hysteric,"  "phobic,"  and  other  clinical  diagnostic  types 
in  terms  of  the  presenting  operations  of  Levels  I  and  II.  It  is  immedi- 
ately possible  to  study  the  behavior  of  these  diagnostic  types  at  the 
other  level  of  personality  and  in  terms  of  the  variability  indices.  How 
do  hysterics  (diagnosed  at  Level  I-M)  see  their  mothers,  fathers,  and 
spouses?  What  are  the  identification-disidentification  indices  for 
schizoids  (diagnosed  at  Level  I-M)?  What  are  the  Level  V  value- 
aspirations  of  obsessives,  hysterics,  etc.?  What  are  the  Level  III 
fantasy  patterns  of  psychosomatics?  The  eight  clinical  chapters  of 
tliis  book  are  entirely  devoted  to  a  consideration  of  these  questions,  to 


234  "^^^  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

a  summary  of  these  new  conceptions  of  diagnosis,  and  to  their  valida- 
tion. 

Thus,  in  linking  interpersonal  terminology  to  psychiatric  diagnoses 
we  have  facilitated  a  systematic  investigation  of  many  important  clini- 
cal problems.  It  will  be  noted,  however,  that  this  research  enterprise 
is  based  on  what  seems  to  be  a  rather  shaky  foundation — the  equiva- 
lence of  the  Kraepehnian-type  diagnosis  to  interpersonal  patterns.  An 
objection  to  this  equation  might  point  out  that  the  relationship  of 
MMPI  diagnostic  patterns  to  interpersonal  types  is  far  from  being  a 
convincing  validation.  Even  though  the  MMPI  is  one  of  the  most  ac- 
cepted and  popular  diagnostic  tests,  and  even  though  it  is  based  on 
carefully  diagnosed  criterion  groups,  this  one  test  cannot  be  considered 
a  satisfactory  criterion  of  psychiatric  diagnosis.  This  objection  is  well 
taken.  This  is  not  a  satisfactory  criterion — but  it  still  stands  as  the  best 
criterion.  The  unhappy  fact  is  that  there  is  no  possibility  of  getting  a 
watertight  estimate  of  Kraepelinian-type  diagnoses.  Like  so  many 
other  complex,  multilevel  concepts  in  psychiatry  the  diagnostic  cate- 
gories, because  of  unreliabiUty  and  subjectivity  of  conception,  possess 
no  standard  criterion  value. 

Five  years  of  experience  in  applying  the  interpersonal  system  to 
clinical  problems,  plus  the  validating  evidence  from  MMPI  studies, 
have  led  to  the  conclusion  that  the  relationships  presented  in  Table  9 
provide  a  satisfactory  functional  definition  of  these  six  psychiatric 
categories.  In  a  later  section  of  this  book  eight  chapters  will  be  de- 
voted to  the  eight  basic  interpersonal  diagnostic  types.  As  we  take  up 
each  adjustive-maladjustive  mode,  we  shall  review  the  Hterature  per- 
taining to  the  psychiatric  equivalents.  At  that  time  it  will  be  suggested 
that  the  standard  clinical  definitions  of  these  six  Kraepelinian-type 
categories  do  involve  interpersonal  factors  that  tend  to  substantiate  the 
relationships  from  our  MMPI  studies  which  are  summarized  in 
Table  9. 

The  logic  of  operational  definition,  we  recall,  allows  the  scientist  to 
define  his  concepts  in  terms  of  his  measurements.  Because  of  the  use- 
fulness of  psychiatric  diagnostic  terminology,  and  its  implicit  inter- 
personal connotations,  it  has  seemed  valuable  to  include  it  within  the 
interpersonal  diagnostic  system.  The  relationships  presented  in  Table 
9  therefore  stand  as  operational  definitions  of  the  psychiatric  terms 
concerned  (at  the  designated  level).  In  the  subsequent  chapters  we 
shall  be  employing  the  terms  "overconventional,"  "dependent,"  etc. 
When  they  are  used  it  will  be  understood  that  they  correspond  to  the 
respective  psychiatric  equivalents  (at  Level  I-M  or  II-C)  as  indicated 
in  Table  9. 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  235 

The  Use  of  Standard  Psychiatric  Terms  in  Interpersonal  Diagnosis 

The  Kraepelinian-type  categories  are  taken  to  be  synonymous  for 
the  appropriate  maladjustive  types.  According  to  this  system  hysteric 
is  a  synonym  for  over  conventional  personality;  obsessive  can  be  used 
interchangeably  with  masochistic  personality .  The  Kraepelinian-type 
terms  do  not  interchange  with  the  adjustive  types,  but  only  with  the 
maladjustive  types. 

The  new  clinical  terminology  is  employed  in  exactly  the  same  man- 
ner as  the  interpersonal  maladaptive  categories.  If  a  patient  manifests 
extreme  LM  at  Level  I  and  extreme  FG  at  Level  II,  he  is  designated  in 
interpersonal  terminology  as  an  overconventional-distrustfjil  personal- 
ity (numerical  code  =  74).  For  research  purposes  we  employ  the  nu- 
merical code.  If  we  wanted  to  communicate  with  a  future  therapist 
who  is  familiar  with  the  interpersonal  system  we  would  probably  use 
the  interpersonal  terms.  If  we  wanted  to  communicate  with  a  con- 
ventionally trained  psychiatrist  who  is  unfamiliar  with  the  inter- 
personal language  we  would  diagnose  the  patient  hysterical-schizoid 
personality . 

Two  additional  considerations  remain  before  concluding  this  chap- 
ter on  interpersonal  diagnosis.  Table  9  presents  the  eight  basic  inter- 
personal types  and  lists  many  standard  psychiatric  categories  which 
seem  to  relate  to  them.  According  to  this  system  of  diagnosis,  phobics, 
neurasthenics,  reactive  depressives,  and  anxiety  neurotics  all  can  be  ex- 
pected to  manifest  docile-dependent  trends.  When  the  diagnostic 
label  is  tied  to  the  security  operations  displayed  by  the  patient,  some 
changes  in  meaning  and  accompanying  paradoxes  can  be  expected.  It 
is  possible  for  a  patient  to  exhibit  a  phobic  or  anxiety  symptom  and 
not  the  dependent  behavior  that  we  expect  to  go  along  with  it.  Some- 
times a  most  aggressive  or  boastfully  self-confident  patient  comes  seek- 
ing psychiatric  help  for  phobic  complaints,  i.e.,  irrational  fears  or  anx- 
iety reactions.  We  diagnose,  however,  on  the  basis  of  character  or 
symptomatic  pressure,  not  on  the  basis  of  the  symptom  itself.  The 
diagnosis  summarizes  the  presenting  operations.  In  cases  of  this  sort  we 
generally  find  that  the  symptoms  are  a  result  of  some  threat  to  the 
overt  security  operations.  Sometimes  the  underlying  operations  reflect 
the  interpersonal  themes  that  go  with  the  symptom.  Thus  the  pa- 
tient who  presents  (at  Levels  I-M  and  II-C)  as  a  counterphobic,  self- 
satisfied  person  with  a  phobic  symptom  would  be  given  the  present- 
ing diagnosis  of  competitive  or  narcissistic  personality  {22  or  22). 
Examination  of  his  "preconscious"  behavior  might  reveal  fearful 
and  dependent  themes  (which  we  have  seen  to  be  related  to  phobic 
material). 


236  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

The  symptom  can  be  related  to  any  level  of  personality,  or  it  can 
simply  reflect  an  environmental  pressure  which  makes  the  overt  se- 
curity operations  inadequate  or  inappropriate.  An  example  might  be  an 
acute  anxiety  attack  suffered  by  a  compulsive,  managerial  personality. 
If  the  multilevel  profile  involves  "preconscious"  passivity  (e.g.,  1 166), 
we  might  surmise  that  the  symptom  represents  a  leaking  out  of  the 
underlying  fear.  If  the  multilevel  pattern  involves  a  solid  four-layer 
edifice  of  strength  and  power  (e.g.,  1111),  then  the  symptom  would 
undoubtedly  be  a  reaction  to  an  environmental  pressure  for  which  his 
compulsive,  managerial  operations  are  inappropriate  or  inadequate. 
The  pedantic,  compulsive  professor  may  be  threatened  by  the  loss  of 
his  job;  or  the  bossy,  self-made  businessman  may  be  threatened  by  the 
prospect  of  failure.  Systematic  diagnosis  cannot,  therefore,  allow  itself 
to  be  tied  to  description  of  symptom.  The  solution,  we  submit,  is  sys- 
tematically to  describe  and  summarize  behavior  at  the  levels  in  which  it 
is  manifested. 

A  final  point  merits  comment.  In  looking  over  the  list  of  standard 
psychiatric  diagnostic  terms  to  which  we  have  given  interpersonal  re- 
definition, it  will  be  noted  that  a  few  common  categories  are  omitted. 
Among  the  terms  which  are  left  out  of  the  list  of  maladjustments  are 
such  familiar  labels  as  manic-depressive,  paranoia,  catatonia,  etc. 

These  have  been  excluded  because  there  seems  to  be  no  typical 
interpersonal  pattern  associated  with  them.  The  essence  of  these  dis- 
orders is  an  inconsistent  behavior.  The  emphasis,  on  the  contrary, 
seems  to  be  on  the  changeability  of  behavior.  This  is  obviously  true  of 
the  hyphenated  term  manic-depressive — the  variability  is  the  essence 
of  the  personality.  The  term  catatonic  (which  seems,  by  the  way, 
vaguely  defined  and  diminishing  in  popular  usage)  is  generally 
described  in  variability  terms.  Paranoid,  however,  seems  on  the  sur- 
face to  have  a  most  clear  interpersonal  meaning.  It  is  used,  in  fact, 
synonymously  with  the  distrustful,  suspicious  personality .  While  not 
everyone  would  agree,  we  have  come  to  the  conclusion  that  paranoid 
is  one  of  the  most  loosely  defined  words  in  the  psychiatric  dictionary. 
A  brief  review  of  its  connotations  will  reveal  its  protean  and  paradoxi- 
cal complexity.  We  have  already  mentioned  its  denotation  of  suspi- 
cion (FG).  It  also  refers  to  delusions  of  grandeur  (BC).  Paranoids 
are  generally  associated  with  litigations,  quarrelsomeness,  or  dangerous 
outbursts  of  aggression  (DE).  They  often  present  themselves  as 
pedantic  and  domineering  (AP).  They  often  claim  to  be  hypernormal 
— denying  pathology  and  weakness  (NO).  They  are  most  frequently 
characterized  by  an  obtuse,  self-righteous,  bland  overconventionality, 
which  Robert  E.  Harris  has  called  "poignant  naivete"  (LM,  JK). 

It  is  clear  that  there  are  a  variety  of  specific  interpersonal  and 


A  SYSTEM  OF  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS 


^37 


symptomatic  pressures  that  the  so-called  "paranoid"  puts  on  the  chni- 
cian.  In  fact,  it  seems  that  the  essence  of  the  term  implies  a  complex, 
unapproachable,  unstable  personality.  Many  clinicians  tend  to  sniff 
out  the  so-called  "paranoid  reaction"  whenever  they  have  the  uneasy 
sense  that  multiplicity  (and  perhaps  duplicity)  of  motive  exists  in  the 
person  they  are  dealing  with.  The  essence  of  this  diagnosis  might  be 
reduced  to  these  factors:  (1)  complexity  and  variety  at  the  level  of 
presentation,  (2)  underlying  hostility  and  distrust  (which  cannot  be 
included  in  the  diagnostic  summaries  of  presenting  operations),  and 
(3)  duplicity  and/or  self-deception  (i.e.,  discrepancy  between  self- 
perception  and  view  of  self  by  others).  These  three  criteria  seem  to 
suggest  that  the  diagnosis  of  paranoia  revolves  around  a  certain  pattern 
of  variability  (especially  conflict  between  presenting  operations  versus 
underlying  themes,  and  between  the  two  types  of  presenting  opera- 
tions [Level  I-M  versus  Level  II-C]). 

For  these  reasons  the  psychiatric  terms  paranoid,  manic-depressive, 
and  catatonic  are  not  considered  as  denoting  interpersonal  patterns  of 
presenting  operations.  They  seem,  instead,  to  p6int  to  certain  phe- 
nomena of  changeability,  conflict,  cyclical  oscillation,  and  mispercep- 
tion. 

The  Diagnostic  Continuum.  In  the  preceding  section  we  have 
cited  evidence  relating  the  interpersonal  types  to  standard  psychiatric 
diagnoses.  The  interpersonal  types  are  on  a  continuum  in  such  a  way 
that  neighboring  behaviors  are  related,  and  behaviors  opposite  on  the 
circle  are  considered  to  be  negatively  related. 

When  we  substitute  psychiatric  diagnostic  terms  for  interpersonal 
categories  we  are  suggesting  that  a  diagnostic  continuum  exists. 

The  advantages  of  such  a  continuum  (if  valid)  are  considerable. 
The  process  of  diagnosis  can  be  changed  from  a  hit-or-miss  pigeon- 
hole classification  to  a  more  systematic  enterprise.  The  reliability  and 
meaning  of  diagnosis  can  be  increased.  If  one  cHnician  using  the 
standard  nosology  calls  a  patient  an  hysteric,  and  a  second  labels  him 
as  phobic,  a  complete  diagnostic  "miss"  must  be  registered.  The  use  of 
the  diagnostic  continuum  can  clarify  this  situation.  In  this  illustrative 
situation  the  two  diagnosticians  would  be  considered  to  be  in  fairly 
close  agreement  since  they  are  just  one  unit  off  in  their  disagreement 
(since  hysteric  is  one  unit  removed  from  phobic  on  the  continuum). 

The  value  of  this  system  depends,  of  course,  on  its  validity.  We 
have  listed  the  diagnoses  in  a  rough  ordinal  array.  Does  this  make 
clinical,  empirical  sense?  Are  hysterics  closer  to  phobics  than  they  are 
to  obsessives? 

The  evidence  from  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  seems  to  con- 
firm the  hypothesis  that  they  are.  This  research,  however,  is  based  on 


238  THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIMENSION 

measurements  which  are  shallow  and  obviously  not  as  broad  or  deep 
as  clinical  impressions. 

The  validity  of  this  diagnostic  continuum  can  be  checked  by  the 
reader  with  clinical  experience  who  can  determine  if  the  ordering  of 
categories  correlates  with  his  diagnostic  experience. 

The  meaning  of  the  diagnostic  continuum  can  be  broken  down  as 
follows:  Psychopathic  personalities  are  held  to  be  closest  to  schizoids. 
They  both  share  the  alienation  and  isolation  from  conventional  be- 
havior. The  former  are  more  active  in  their  hostihty,  the  latter  more 
passive. 

Schizoid  personalities  are  also  close  to  obsessives.  They  both  share 
a  pessimistic,  self-derogatory  attitude.  The  former  are  more  bitter  and 
distrustful,  the  latter  are  more  self-derogatory  and  worried. 

Obsessive  personalities  are  also  close  to  phobics.  They  both  share  a 
depressed,  worried  passivity.  The  former  are  more  guilty,  more  aware 
of  their  emotions.  The  latter  fail  to  recognize  the  emotional  sources 
of  their  condition  and  are  more  concerned  with  symptoms  external  to 
their  character  structure. 

Phobic  persoTialities  are  also  close  to  hysterics.  They  both  share  a 
repressive,  conventional  facade.  They  both  externahze  and  tend  to  be 
unaware  of  specific  interpersonal  problems.  The  former  are  more  fear- 
ful and  worried,  the  latter  are  more  bland  and  unworried. 

The  hysterical  personality  is  also  close  to  the  psychosomatic  adjust- 
ment. They  both  share  conventional  operations  and  claim  to  be  un- 
worried and  sound  "psychologically."  The  former  are  more  aware  of 
some  symptomatic  "tension";  they  present  physical  symptoms  which 
are  directly  symbolic  of  underlying  emotions.  They  are  also  relatively 
more  passive  and  conciliatory.  The  psychosomatic  personality  em- 
phasizes more  activity  and  hypernormal  responsibility  than  the  hys- 
teric. 

The  managerial  and  narcissistic  personality  types  are  not  usually 
considered  standard  diagnostic  categories  and  will  therefore  be 
omitted  from  these  comparisons. 

Rejerences 

1.  AscH,  p.  The  reliability  of  psychiatric  diagnoses.  /.  abn.  soc.  Psychol.,  1949,  44, 
272-76. 

2.  DoERiNG,  C.  R.  Reliability  of  observation  of  psychiatric  and  related  characteristics. 
Amer.  J.  Orthophyschiat.,  1934,  4,  249-57. 

3.  Elfin,  F.  Specialists  interpret  the  case  of  Harold  Holzer.  /.  abn.  soc.  Psychol., 
1947,42,  99-111. 

4.  Harris,  R.  E.  Psychodiagnostic  testing  in  psychiatry  and  psychosomatic  medicine. 
In  Recent  advances  in  diagnostic  psychological  testing.  Springfield,  III.:  Charles  C. 
Thomas,  1950. 

5.  Masserman,  J.  H.,  and  H.  T.  Carmichael.  Diagnosis  and  prognosis  in  psychiatry. 
/.  Ment.  Sci.,  1938,  S4,  893-946. 


Ill 

The  Variability  Dimension  of  Personality: 
Theory  and  Variables 


Introduction 


The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  works  within  the  scope  of 
two  areas  of  personality — the  interpersonal  and  variability  dimensions. 
In  Part  II  we  presented  the  five  levels  at  which  we  measure  inter- 
personal behavior,  and  a  multilevel  system  of  interpersonal  diagnosis 
was  described. 

This  section  of  the  book  presents  an  over-all  view  of  the  variability 
dimension  and  the  Kaiser  Foundation  theory  of  variability. 

The  variables  by  which  we  measure  conflict  and  interlevel  dis- 
crepancy are  called  variability  indices.  These  are  the  variables  of  per- 
sonality organization — which  relate  behavior  at  different  levels.  Chap- 
ter 1 3  presents  operational  definitions  of  forty-eight  indices  of  varia- 
bility. Some  of  these  interlevel  relationships  are  like  classic  psycho- 
analytic defense  mechanisms.  These  indices  are  described  and  defined 
because  we  shall  be  employing  them  in  the  subsequent  descriptions  of 
clinical  and  diagnostic  types.  Their  detailed  description,  validation, 
and  clinical  applicability  will  not  be  included  in  this  book. 

Our  present  purpose  is  to  outline  a  system  of  interpersonal  diagnosis 
and  the  variables  by  which  this  is  accomplished.  Some  of  the  research 
findings  which  involve  variability  indices  (e.g.,  identification  and  mis- 
perception)  are  summarized  in  the  diagnostic  section  (Chapters  14 
through  23).  Other  descriptions  and  validations  of  variability  indices 
have  been  published  in  scientific  journals  (I,  2). 

The  chapter  to  follow  will,  therefore,  be  restricted  to  a  brief  discus- 
sion of  theory  and  a  listing  of  the  indices  of  variability. 

References 

1.  LaForge,  R.,  T.  Leary,  H.  Naboisek,  H.  Coffey,  and  M.  Freedman.  The  inter- 
personal dimension  of  personality:  II.  An  objective  study  of  repression.  /.  Pers., 
1954,  23,  2,  129-53. 

2.  Leary,  T.,  and  H.  Coffey.  The  prediction  of  interpersonal  behavior  in  group 
psychotherapy.  Fsychodrama  Group  Psychother.  Mojiogr.,  1955,  No.  28. 


240 


13 


The  Indices  of  Variability 


The  preceding  pages  of  this  book — Chapters  1  through  12 — have  con- 
sidered the  interpersonal  dimension  of  personality.  We  have  seen  that 
security  operations  can  be  classified  in  terms  of  sixteen  variables.  We 
have  also  considered  the  fact  that  interpersonal  behavior  exists  at  dif- 
ferent levels  and  that  these  levels  may  be  defined  in  terms  of  the  source 
and  nature  of  their  expression. 

The  interpersonal  dimension  has  five  levels.  A  circular  continuum 
of  variables  is  employed  for  all  measures  of  emotional  behavior.  We 
have  dealt  in  some  detail  with  the  measurements,  meaning,  and  func- 
tion of  these  five  levels.  In  presenting  this  material,  the  levels  were 
considered  separately.  The  point  was  made  repeatedly  that  the  data 
from  any  level  are  most  useful  in  relation  to  all  the  other  levels.  But  so 
far  we  have  concentrated  on  statements  about  each  level  in  isolation, 
because  we  have  had  no  systematic  way  of  dealing  with  the  dynamic 
interplay  among  levels.  The  time  has  come  to  discuss  these  relation- 
ships. We  are  going  to  fit  together  four^  discrete  parts  of  personality 
structure  in  order  to  build  a  systematic  theory  of  personality  organi- 
zation. We  are  going  to  study  the  integration  of  the  over-all  personal- 
ity. The  shift  in  reference  we  make  here  is  important  to  note.  Up 
until  now  we  have  dealt  with  unilevel  data.  Now  we  are  going  to 
study  multilevel  phenomena — the  dynamics  of  organization.  It  is 
necessary,  at  this  point,  to  distinguish  between  statements  that  refer  to 
security  operations  and  to  the  arrangement  of  interpersonal  variables 
at  a  single  level  or  area  (i.e.,  the  circle)  and  those  which  refer  to  the 
differences  among  the  circles.  The  former  can  be  called  class  (i.e., 
unilevel)  statements  and  the  latter  relationship  (i.e.,  multilevel)  state- 
ments. 

When  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  an  individual  at  one  level  of 
personality  is  classified,  the  resulting  data  might  take  the  form  of  the 
following:  "The  patient  complains  to  the  therapist."  "He  attacks  the 

^  Level  IV  not  included. 

241 


2^2  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 

other  group  members."  These  are  class  statements.  They  refer  to  one 
area  of  security  operations — Level  I  Communications.  A  patient  says, 
"I  like  my  therapist  and  the  other  group  patients."  These  are  also  class 
statements.  They  refer  to  another  area  of  behavior — Level  II,  Con- 
scious Descriptions. 

When  behavior  at  one  level  is  compared  to  behavior  at  another 
level,  relationship  statements  are  being  made.  The  discrepancies  be- 
tween levels  become  the  focus.  We  note,  for  example,  that  a  patient 
consciously  reports  himself  as  friendly,  although  his  behavior  as  rated 
by  others  is  hostile.  We  might  say  that  this  patient  misperceives  his 
hostile  behavior.  The  word  Tmsperceives  is  a  relationship  term  since  it 
compares  two  levels  of  observation — the  subjective  and  objective 
view  of  the  patient's  behavior. 

When  we  shift  from  sentences  about  a  single  level  to  sentences 
about  the  differences  between  levels,  a  new  set  of  concepts  is  involved. 
The  term  misperceives  is  an  example  of  such  a  concept,  A  new  vocab- 
ulary and  syntax  come  into  play.  This  comes  under  the  heading  of 
the  "Logic  of  Levels."  The  distinction  between  class  and  relationship 
statements  must  be  kept  clear  or  faulty  conceptualization  will  result. 

What  Is  the  Variability  Dimension? 

We  are  dealing  here  with  an  entirely  new  and  different  type  of 
data — the  relationships  among  the  levels  of  personality.  We  designate 
this  as  the  variability  dimension  of  personality.  This  is  a  most  impor- 
tant aspect  of  behavior.  Variability  has  classically  been  the  stumbling 
block  in  the  development  of  personality  theory.  Every  systematic 
treatment  of  human  nature  has  had  to  labor  with  the  perverse  incon- 
sistency of  behavior.  It  has  never  been  difficult  for  theorists  to  invent 
typologies  and  variables  of  emotions.  The  trouble  has  always  come 
when  the  elusive  human  subject  begins  to  demonstrate  his  protean 
complexity. 

The  first  theoretical  lever  which  succeeded  in  moving  this  obstacle 
was  provided  by  the  theory  of  unconscious  motivation.  When  Sig- 
mund  Freud  defined  the  multilevel  nature  of  personality,  he  offered  the 
first  systematic  explanation  of  conflicts,  ambivalence,  and  incon- 
sistency. The  essence  of  a  dynamic  psychology  is  variability.  The 
great  advantage  of  a  depth  theory  is  that  it  explains  the  puzzling  com- 
plexity and  contradiction  inherent  in  human  behavior.  The  psycho- 
analytic theory  of  personality  and  of  neurosis  is  defined  in  terms  of 
interlevel  conflict,  i.e.,  variability.  There  are  certain  motives  at  one 
level  and  certain  other  motives  at  another.  Their  interplay  spells  out 
the  organization  of  personality.   When  Freud  presents  his  great  tri- 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  243 

partite  division  of  character  into  ego,  superego,  and  id,  he  is  mainly 
concerned  with  conceptualizing  the  multilevel  variability  of  behavior. 
VariabiUty  is  involved  in  almost  every  aspect  of  personality  that  we 
study.  Conflict,  ambivalence,  defense  mechanisms,  growth,  regression, 
change,  improvement-in-therapy — all  these  phenomena  have  the  basic 
factor  in  common — one  unit  of  measure  varies  in  relation  to  another. 
It  is  a  major  thesis  of  this  book  that  all  change  phenomena  are,  to  a 
certain  extent,  functions  of  a  general  rigidity-oscillation  factor.  This 
factor  is  measurable  and  predictable  in  terms  of  the  time  and  the 
amount  of  variability.  Some  human  beings  are  more  variable,  some  are 
less.  Some  express  variation  between  certain  areas  of  their  personality; 
others  express  it  in  different  areas.  The  amount  and  kind  of  variability 
is  a  most  significant  variable  of  human  behavior.  It  defines  the  type 
and  intensity  of  conflict.  It  determines  the  tendency  to  change  or  to 
maintain  a  rigid  adjustment.  It  becomes  a  key  variable  in  the  diag- 
nostic and  prognostic  formulas  through  which  we  conceptualize  hu- 
man personality. 

Structural,  Temporal,  and  Situational  Variability 

In  considering  the  variability  factor  it  is  useful  to  make  the  follow- 
ing distinctions  between  structural,  temporal,  and  situational  variabil- 
ity. 

Structural  variability  refers  to  differences  among  the  levels  of  per- 
sonality. It  is  well  known  that  drastic  discrepancies  and  inconsistencies 
develop  when  we  compare  the  conscious  self-description  with  be- 
havioral or  symbolic  expressions.  The  subject  who  presents  himself  as 
a  warm-hearted,  tender  soul  may  produce  dreams  or  fantasies  which 
are  bitterly  murderous.  Social  interactions,  as  observed  by  others,  may 
be  quite  different  from  the  subject's  own  view  of  them. 

Temporal  variability  refers  to  inconsistencies  in  the  same  level  of 
behavior  over  a  time  span.  Time  inevitably  brings  changes,  great  or 
small.  Many  subjects  show  marked  cyclical  swings  of  mood  or  ac- 
tion. The  interpersonal  behavior  of  an  individual  generally  mutates 
as  he  moves  from  age  13  to  31.  The  temporal  changes  we  study  in 
psychiatric  patients  are  called  spontaneous  remissions,  therapeutic  re- 
coveries, psychotic  episodes,  and  the  like. 

Situational  variability  refers  to  differences  in  cultural  and  environ- 
mental factors.  The  man  who  is  a  lion  at  home  may  be  a  lamb  in  the 
office.  Reactions  often  vary  according  to  the  sex,  age,  and  cultural 
status  of  the  "other  one"  with  whom  the  subject  is  dealing. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project  is  studying  the  hypothesis 
that  all  of  these  types  of  changes  are  related  to  the  same  variability 


244  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 

factor.  To  distinguish  between  them  may  be  an  artifact,  operationally- 
useful  in  the  light  of  the  scientific  manageability  of  change  phenomena. 
It  is  difficult  enough  to  measure  interpersonal  behavior  at  one  time 
and  at  one  level.  To  study  variation  it  is  necessary  to  have  two  sets  of 
data  on  the  same  subject  which  we  compare.  Three  distinct  operations 
are  required.  We  must  measure  one  set  of  behaviors,  then  the  other, 
and  finally,  the  discrepancy  or  change  index.  We  can  isolate  tem- 
poral variation  by  holding  constant  the  level  and  the  situation  from 
which  the  data  come,  and  measuring  the  change  over  time.  Repeating 
the  same  personality  test  on  a  control  patient  might  be  an  example  of 
this  type  of  variation.  Observing  in  a  group  therapy  session  a  patient's 
successive  reactions  to  a  nurturant  therapist  and  a  competitive  fellow 
patient  would  illustrate  a  change  in  the  interpersonal  situation  often 
leading  to  variance  in  the  subject's  responses.  If  we  establish  the  kind 
of  variability  in  one  of  these  classes  (e.g.,  in  personality  structure)  we 
can  make  probability  predictions  as  to  the  kind  of  variation  to  be  ex- 
pected in  another  class. 

Comparison  Between  the  Interpersonal 
and  the  Variability  Dimension 

The  network  of  relationships  of  the  various  parts  of  the  system — 
and  that  is  what  we  deal  with  here — obviously  tells  us  something  about 
the  organization  of  that  system.  Interlevel  discrepancies  are  therefore 
indices  of  organization.  They  tell  us  about  the  agreement  or  conflict 
between  the  various  levels  of  behavior.  They  tell  us  not  how  the  sub- 
ject relates  to  his  environment,  but  rather  how  the  different  areas  of 
his  personality  relate  to  each  other.  In  the  interpersonal  dimension 
we  study  different  phenomena  and  employ  different  variables.  For  ex- 
ample, the  subject's  Level  I  profile  is  measured  in  terms  of  the  sixteen 
interpersonal  variables,  summarizing  his  actual  relationships  with  other 
people.  The  Level  II  profile  employs  the  same  sixteen  variables  to 
summarize  his  consciously  described  relationships  with  other  people. 
When  we  compare  the  Level  II  and  III  profiles  we  get  a  discrepancy 
score.  We  move  into  a  new  dimension  and  must  employ  a  new  set  of 
variables. 

These  variables  are  not  interpersonal,  but  intrapersonal.  Projec- 
tion and  suppression  are  terms  used  to  describe  certain  kinds  of  rela- 
tionship between  levels  of  personality  data.  Notice  that  they  are  not 
directly  interpersonal;  one  does  not  project  or  suppress  another  per- 
son. He  projects  or  suppresses  his  own  private  motivation.  These  re- 
lationship variables  refer  not  to  his  social  relationships  but  to  the  rela- 
tionships which  hold  between  the  areas  of  his  own  behavior.  Similarly, 
the  rating  of  amount  of  interlevel  discrepancy  must  be  distinguished 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  245 

from  an  interpersonal  rating.  We  do  not  use  the  terms  rigidity  and 
conflict  to  describe  what  one  person  does  to  another — we  employ  them 
to  describe  the  tightness,  looseness,  consistency,  or  ambivalence  among 
the  levels  of  personality. 

Thus,  we  have  introduced  into  the  interpersonal  system  not  just  a 
new  variable,  but  a  new  category  of  classification.  This  is  called  the 
variability  dimension  of  personality.  A  new  order  of  measurement  is 
involved  which  taps  all  of  the  variability  phenomena  of  human  be- 
havior: similarity-difference,  change,  discrepancy,  conflict.  There  are, 
as  we  have  seen,  many  types  of  changes  that  show  up  in  human  be- 
havior— those  due  to  time,  situation,  and  chance,  as  well  as  those  due 
to  lawful  inconsistencies  among  the  levels  of  personality.  The  vari- 
ability dimension  is  a  formal  aspect  of  personality  as  opposed  to  the 
more  empirical  procedures  by  which  we  measure  interpersonal  be- 
havior. Its  variables  are  determined  not  from  empirical  observation, 
but  from  logical  procedures. 

In  developing  the  interpersonal  variables,  we  began  with  actual 
interpersonal  behavior.  We  collected  emotional  data  of  all  kinds  and 
then  developed  a  system  which  best  reflected  the  varieties  of  inter- 
personal purpose.  In  developing  the  levels  of  personality  the  same  em- 
pirical technique  was  followed.  The  diff^erent  sources  of  data  were 
examined  and  combined  into  the  four  levels.  But  in  determining  the 
measurement  and  conceptual  units  for  the  variability  dimension,  a  dif- 
ferent solution  is  involved.  We  are  not  dealing  with  actual  human  be- 
havior, but  with  indices  of  change — changes  in  the  scores  from  the 
interpersonal  dimension.  The  variables  of  the  variability  dimension  are 
not  units  of  the  subject's  behavior,  but  of  the  scientists'  behavior,  for 
it  is  the  scientist  who  performs  the  operations.^ 

This  is  a  point  worth  stressing.  The  number  and  kind  of  inter- 
personal variables  were  limited  and  determined  only  by  our  observa- 
tions of  what  individuals  do  to  each  other  in  their  social  interaction. 
The  number  of  levels  was  similarly  determined  by  empirical  evidence. 
A  certain  flexibility  in  the  selection  of  variables  and  levels  does  exist 
because  any  scientist  has  the  right  to  increase  or  diminish  the  number 
of  categories  by  which  he  classifies  behavior.  In  setting  up  empirical 
categories,  some  room  for  interpretive  judgment  is  allowed — the  em- 
pirical data  guides,  but  does  not  dictate.  Once  the  number  of  levels  is 
determined,  however,  the  system  becomes  "set."  When  the  scientist 

^  To  be  more  precise  we  should  say  that  the  variability  dimension  is  twice  removed 
from  behavior,  and  the  interpersonal  dimension  is  once  removed.  The  patient  does 
something;  then  the  scientist  categorizes  or  measures  it.  The  interpersonal  data  ob- 
tained in  this  way  are  once  removed  from  the  subject's  behavior.  Then  the  scientist 
goes  on  to  compare  the  different  levels  of  areas  of  interpersonal  data.  These  formal, 
analytic  operations  are  thus  twice  removed  from  raw  behavior. 


246 


THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


goes  on  to  compare  the  differences  between  the  levels  he  has  selected, 
there  is  no  longer  any  freedom  for  interpretation  or  creative  choice. 
The  logic  of  levels  takes  over  and  dictates  the  range  and  nature  of  the 
interlevel  discrepancies. 

An  empirical  system  for  measuring  multilevel  behavior  leads,  then, 
to  a  somewhat  new  theory  of  personality  organization.  A  different 
conception  of  conflict  is  defined,  not  in  terms  of  the  interplay  between 
postulated  forces,  but  in  terms  of  the  discrepancies  between  measures. 

The  Indices  of  Variability 

The  relationships  between  areas  of  personality  are  called  variability 
indices.  We  have  already  discussed  their  general  similarity  to  the  con- 
ception of  (but  not  the  clinical  use  of)  Freudian  defense  mechanisms. 
These  indices  comprise  one  aspect  of  the  variability  dimension  of  per- 
sonality. They  reflect  the  stabihty  or  variation  existing  among  the 
levels  of  personality  at  one  point  in  time.  They  are  to  be  distinguished 
from  other  kinds  of  change  phenomena  included  in  the  variability  di- 
mension, such  as  modulations  over  time  (which  includes  change  in 
psychotherapy)  or  variability  due  to  differences  in  the  cultural  situa- 
tion. We  deal  here  with  indices  of  interlevel  conflicts  and  con- 
cordances. 

The  term  variability  index  has  been  assigned  to  this  kind  of  varia- 
tion for  the  following  reason.  This  is  a  rather  neutral  term.  It  is  in- 
tended to  point  out  that  we  are  not  dealing  with  mechanisms  or  even 
with  behaviors,  but  rather  with  formal,  comparative  operations.  These 
discrepancy  relationships  do  not  "do  anything."  It  is  risky  to  mechan- 
ize or  humanize  them.  There  are  no  body  organs  or  neural  centers  for 
repression  or  suppression. 

From  the  empirical  point  of  view,  there  is  simply  behavior  at  dif- 
ferent, discriminable  levels  of  expression.  There  are  measured  rela- 
tionships between  these  levels.  In  the  present  insecure  state  of  our 
knowledge  it  seems  safest  to  call  them  indices.  But  indices  of  what? 
To  answer  this  question  is  to  produce  a  theory  of  personality  organiza- 
tion. 

Variability  index  is,  we  expect,  a  temporary  holding  term  which 
can  be  replaced  by  a  more  dynamic  term  whenever  the  nature  of  the 
dynamic  principle  is  determined.  In  the  meantime,  it  seems  to  express 
exactly  what  we  know  to  be  true  about  the  interlevel  relationships.  It 
tells  us  how  stable  or  variant  these  relationships  are.  If  the  themes  of 
Level  II  parallel  those  of  Level  III,  then  the  variability  index  is  low. 
The  aggression,  let  us  say,  of  one  level  is  repeated  at  the  other.  If  the 
two  levels  are  discrepant — if,  for  example,  the  aggression  at  Level  II 
changes  to  docile  cooperativeness  at  Level  III — a  high  variability  index 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  247 

is  obtained.  Variation  between  the  levels  is  present.  There  is  another 
and  more  important  aspect  of  variability.  There  is  evidence  (see  Ap- 
pendix 3 )  that  the  more  stable  the  organization  of  personality — that  is, 
the  more  the  data  from  Levels  II,  III,  and  I  tend  to  repeat  the  same 
themes — the  less  variation  we  can  expect  in  the  personality  organiza- 
tion over  time.  Conversely,  the  more  conflict  or  oscillation  among  the 
levels  of  personality,  the  more  change  we  can  predict  will  take  place 
in  the  future;  and  this  includes  change  in  therapy.  These  findings 
make  the  term  variability  index  doubly  appropriate.  While  we  can- 
not, at  this  point,  say  that  interlevel  discrepancies  possess  the  dynamic 
qualities  of  pushing  toward  equilibrium,  we  can  say  that  they  refer  to 
structural  stability  of  personality  (this  by  definition),  and  they  predict 
the  degree  of  stability  of  personality  organization  to  be  expected  in 
the  future.  They  give  us,  first  of  all,  an  index  of  systematic  variation 
in  the  personality  structure  at  the  time  of  evaluation,  and  they  point 
out  the  direction  and  amount  of  change  to  be  expected  over  future 
time. 

The  Function  of  Variability  Indices 

The  interpersonal  system  does  not  assign  a  function  to  these  inter- 
level  discrepancies.  Behavior  at  all  levels  is  seen  as  having  one  basic 
function  to  ward  off  survival  anxiety.  The  discrepancies  or  conflicts 
between  levels  are  seen  as  another  dimension  of  conception  which  con- 
cerns the  psychologist's  behavior.  It  is  the  psychologist  who  measures 
the  discrepancy  or  conflict  between  the  two  levels  of  the  patient's  be- 
havior. We  assign,  for  semantic  convenience  and  heuristic  necessity, 
conceptual  titles  to  the  important  discrepancies  between  the  levels  of 
the  subject's  behavior.  We  do  not,  however,  assign  functions  to  them. 
The  only  assumption  upon  which  an  empirical  theory  of  personaUty 
need  be  based  is  the  premise  of  survival  anxiety.  The  only  function 
we  assign  to  behavior  is  the  maintenance  of  security  and  the  diminish- 
ing of  anxiety.  The  indices  of  diff^erence,  ambivalence,  or  conflict 
among  the  varieties  of  behavior  do  not  seem  to  require  the  postulation 
of  additional  functions. 

Closely  connected  to  this  question  of  the  function  of  defense  mech- 
anisms (or  variability  indices)  is  another  issue  which  has  received  con- 
siderable attention  in  the  recent  literature.  This  involves  the  differen- 
tiation between  adaptive  and  defensive  functions.  Are  defense  mech- 
anisms pathological  and  neurotic,  or  can  they  sometimes  be  construc- 
tive? Fenichel,  for  example,  places  all  "successful  defenses"  under  the 
heading  of  sublimation,  and  describes  "unsuccessful  defenses  which 
necessitate  a  repetition  or  perpetuation  of  the  warding-off  process  to 
prevent  the  eruption  of  the  warded-off  impulses."    Mowrer  has  dis- 


248  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 

tinguished  between  the  mechanisms  used  in  development  and  those 
used  in  defense.  This  issue  of  the  adaptability  or  pathology  attached 
to  the  discrepancies  of  concordance  and  conflict  in  personality  is  in- 
variably complicated  by  value  judgments  (e.g.,  what  is  adaptive?)  and 
tlieoretical  assumptions  about  the  function  of  defense  mechanisms. 
These  are  legitimate  questions  from  the  standpoint  of  the  psycho- 
analytic approach  and  deserve  the  attention  they  have  received. 

From  the  position  of  the  interpersonal  system,  this  issue  could  be  in- 
terpreted as  follows:  (1)  the  variabihty  indices  have  no  function;  (2) 
discrepancy  or  conflict  between  levels  cannot  be  assigned  an  adaptive 
or  maladjustive  value  by  definition  but  must  be  interpreted  as  part  of 
the  total  personality  picture.  The  level  and  amount  of  the  conflict  and 
its  relationship  to  the  over-all  character  structure  determine  the  posi- 
tive or  negative  interpretation. 

A  not  infrequent  clinical  misinterpretation  of  psychoanalytic 
theory  implies  that  defense  mechanisms  are  negative  or  neurotic  proc- 
esses. This  is,  indeed,  one  reason  which  supports  the  use  of  the  more 
neutral  term  variability  index  for  the  interlevel  conflicts.  A  discrep- 
ancy between  conscious  self-description  and  "preconscious"  fantasy 
(which  we  shall  designate  repression)  should  not  necessarily  be  con- 
sidered unhealthy.  If  the  Level  II  self-image  is  one-sided  and  the  "pre- 
conscious" fantasy  a  moderate  balance  in  the  opposite  direction,  the 
conflict  might  well  designate  an  adaptive  equilibrium.  If  the  patient  is 
markedly  disidentified  with  his  father,  the  adjustive  aspect  of  this  dis- 
crepancy would  certainly  depend  somewhat  on  the  kind  of  motives 
attributed  to  self,  to  father,  and  to  others. 

In  the  subsequent  pages  we  shall  be  considering  several  variability 
indices  which  have  been  given  names  of  psychoanalytic  defense  mech- 
anisms where  these  seemed  to  fit  the  nature  of  the  conflict.  In  order 
to  understand  the  meaning  and  use  of  these  indices  in  the  interpersonal 
system,  it  is  essential  that  two  points  be  kept  in  mind:  (1)  These  con- 
cepts are  not  mechanisms  or  dynamisms,  but  rather  numerical  indices 
of  interlevel  variation;  as  such  they  have  no  function.  (2)  They  have 
no  a  priori  value-loading  as  far  as  adjustment  and  maladjustment  are 
concerned;  they  can  describe  flexibility  and  healthy  ambivalence,  or 
they  can  indicate  pathological  rigidity  or  maladaptive  oscillation. 

Two  Interpretations  of  Variability 

When  we  obtain  the  variability  indices  among  the  levels  of  per- 
sonality, two  interpretations  of  the  resulting  variation  can  be  made — 
both  of  theoretical  and  practical  interest.  We  can  concentrate  on 
what  the  variation  is,  or  we  can  focus  on  how  Tmich.  The  first  tells  us 
that  the  individual  represses  so  much  hostility  or  misperceives  this 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  249 

much  passivity.  The  second  way  of  handhng  variability  indices  is  to 
disregard  the  content  of  the  interpersonal  themes,  study  the  pattern  of 
variability  for  all  the  discrepancy  relationships,  and  simply  determine 
how  variable  this  person  is  in  over-all  terms.  This  focuses  on  the 
amount  of  variability.  We  can  then  make  such  statements  as,  "This 
patient  is  extremely  conflicted  and  variable,  being  two  sigmas  above 
the  mean." 

We  have  seen  that  the  logic  of  levels  determines  the  kind  of  rela- 
tionships among  levels.  We  have  developed  a  system  in  which  there 
are  eight  general  levels  and  areas  of  personality.  Therefore,  when 
we  ask  the  question,  "What  are  the  relationships  among  the  areas  of 
personality?"  the  answer  is  already  settled  for  us.  They  are  the  rela- 
tionships among  these  eight  areas — the  discrepancies  which  occur 
when  we  compare  each  level  or  sublevel  with  every  other  level. 
Formally,  then,  there  are  as  many  relationships  or  variability  indices 
as  there  are  permutations  among  the  areas. 

Figure  31  presents  these  eight  areas.  Each  circle  represents  a  dis- 
crete area  of  personality  data.  The  lines  joining  the  circles  represent 
the  interlevel  or  interarea  discrepancy  indices  determined  by  the  logic 
of  levels.  They  comprise  the  network  of  variability  indices  which 
link  the  parts  of  personality  structure  into  an  organized  totality. 
Validation  of  these  indices  is  beyond  the  scope  of  this  book.  Thirteen 
of  these  relationships — those  most  relevant  to  current  theory  and 
clinical  practice — are  defined  in  this  chapter. 

The  Record  Booklet  for  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Personality 
(Appendix  4,  Figure  61)  provides  a  simplified  method  for  measuring 
discrepancy  indices  and  for  plotting  them  in  diagrammatic  summary 
form. 

The  next  task  is  to  determine  the  meaning  of  these  interlevel  rela- 
tionships. The  subtractive  procedures,  it  will  be  recalled,  indicate 
the  kind  and  amount  of  interpersonal  behavior  in  one  area  that  is  pres- 
ent in  another  area.  Giving  names  to  these  relationships  is,  in  one  way, 
the  simplest  problem  of  all.  The  term  which  best  mirrors  the  rela- 
tionship is  selected  and  operationally  defined  in  terms  of  the  cross-level 
subtraction.  A  procedure  of  this  sort  satisfies  all  the  logical  require- 
ments, but  the  reader  is  likely  to  remain  unsatisfied  and  to  ask  the 
further  questions,  "This  is  all  very  well,  but  what  do  they  mean? 
What  is  their  functional  value?   What  do  they  predict?" 

A  scientific  system  can  be  objective  and  logically  virtuous  and  still 
have  no  function  except  perhaps  to  entertain  the  originator.  It  would 
be  possible  to  assign  very  impressive  terms  to  the  interlevel  relation- 
ships, calling  this  one  "repression"  and  that  one  "displacement"  and  a 
third  "introjection,"  etc.,  until  the  long  list  of  relationships  (or  the 


250 


THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY 


251 


imagination)  is  exhausted.  The  resulting  nomenclature  would  be 
logically  consistent  and  objective  (since  all  the  terms  would  be  opera- 
tionally defined),  but,  what  is  rather  unfortunate,  it  would  be  quite 
irrelevant.  In  developing  a  system  of  personality,  the  first  problem 
is  the  selection  of  the  categories.  The  next  is  to  validate  them,  that  is, 
to  relate  them  to  other  independent  and  relevant  variables,  to  harness 
them  to  functionally  useful  predictions.  The  three  criteria  for  effec- 
tive research,  we  recall,  are  objective  measurement,  logical  analysis, 
and,  far  from  the  least  important,  functional  relevance. 

If  it  were  feasible  to  list  and  label  all  the  possible  interlevel  relation- 
ships, the  next  task  would  be  to  validate  them  against  functional 
criteria.  Such  labeling  and  validating  would  place  an  enormous  drain 
on  inventive  imagination,  research  resources,  and  reader  endurance 
alike.  At  this  point,  we  shall  attempt  to  define  twelve  generic  varia- 
bility indices,  and  then  list  forty-eight  specific  indices  which  fall  into 
the  twelve  broader  categories. 

The  twelve  variability  indices  about  to  be  defined  have  been  chosen 
because  they  appear  to  possess  the  most  clinical  meaning,  functional 
value,  and  theoretical  implication.  As  we  begin  this  exercise  in  the 
mathematics  of  personality,  it  is  well  to  keep  in  mind  the  formal  or 
logical  aspect  of  the  task.  In  one  sense,  it  is  not  absolutely  necessary 
to  develop  a  notational  system  for  linking  up  the  levels  of  personality. 
In  our  diagnostic  procedures  we  could  conceivably  just  present  the 
interpersonal  behavior  at  all  levels.  We  would  indicate  that  the  sub- 
ject is  hostile  at  Level  I,  claims  to  be  docile  at  Level  II,  describes  his 
father  as  autocratic  at  Level  II  Other,  etc.  The  language  of  variability 
allows  us  to  relate  these  areas  or  levels  of  personality.  It  allows  us  to 
define  systematically  the  dynamic  network  which  links  up  the  de- 
scribed areas.  This  is  a  great  convenience.  Like  any  formal,  notational 
device,  the  language  of  variability  makes  possible  concise,  precise  sum- 
maries of  conflict,  concordance,  discrepancy,  etc. 

Operatioiial  Definition  of  the  Variability  Indices 

There  are  twelve  generic  variability  indices  to  be  defined  and 
validated  in  this  chapter.  Most  of  these  generic  discrepancies  have 
several  subdivisions  which  are  specific  indices  referring  to  the  impor- 
tant familial  figures  to  which  the  subject  is  related.  Thus,  under  the 
generic  index  conscious  identification  there  are  four  specific  indices 
referring  to  identifications  with  father,  mother,  spouse,  and  therapist. 
Table  10  presents  the  twelve  generic  variability  indices  and  indicates 
the  subvarieties  which  are  subsumed  under  this  general  title. 

In  the  left  column  of  Table  10  are  listed  the  most  familiar  titles  of 
the  twelve  generic  variability  indices.   In  the  right-hand  column  are 


2J2  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


TABLE  10 

Informal  Listing  of  the  T'welve  Generic  Variability  Indices 

Code  Number  of  the  Specific  Variability 
Title  of  Variability  Indices  Indices  Subsumed  Under  this  General  Title 

Role  Coincidence  11  SO 

Interpersonal  Perception  12  SS,  12  OO 

Conscious  Identification  22  SM,  22  SF,  22  SSp,  22  ST 

Equation  22  MF,  22  MSp,  22  FSp,  22  MT,  22  FT,  22  SpT 

Repression  23  SH 

Cross-level  Identification  23  MH,  23  FH,  23  SpH,  23  TH,  23  SO,  23  SM, 

23  SF,  23  SSp 

Conscious-"Preconscious"  Fusion  23  MM,  23  FF,  23  SpSp 

Displacement  23  A'lF,  23  FM,  Z3SpM,  23  SpF 

23  M  Sp,  23  F  Sp,  23  TM,  23  TF,  23  TSp 

"Preconscious"  Identification  33  HM,  33  HF,  33  HSp,  33  HO 

Self-Acceptance  25  IS 

Conscious  Idealization  25  IM,  25  IF,  25  ISp,  25  IT 

"Preconscious"  Idealization  35  IH,  35  IM,  35  IF,  35lSp,  35  lO 

noted  the  code  designations  of  the  specific  variability  indices  which 
are  the  subvarieties  of  the  generic  indices.  It  will  be  observed  that 
there  are  forty-eight  of  these  specific  indices.  All  of  these  will  be 
operationally  defined  in  the  subsequent  pages. 

It  will  be  noted  that  many  of  these  generic  variability  indices  have 
been  given  the  names  of  classical  psychoanalytic  defense  mechanisms. 
Although  borrowing  terminology  from  another  theory  has  its  risks, 
we  have  ventured  to  employ  the  familiar  terms  wherever  they  seem  to 
fit  the  general  nature  of  the  discrepancy  concerned.  In  this  manner 
we  have  sought  to  avoid  the  proliferation  of  novel  terms  and  the 
idiosyncratic  "timid  neologisms"  which  Egon  Brunswick  has  deplored. 
Several  of  the  discrepancy  indices,  however,  involve  reflex  inter- 
personal communications — a  level  of  personality  which  has  not  been 
isolated  by  the  psychoanalytic  theory.  This  has  necessitated  the  intro- 
duction of  some  new  terminology — or  in  Freudian  language,  some 
new  "defense  mechanisms." 

Confusion  between  these  variability  indices  and  psychoanalytic 
mechanisms  of  the  same  name  may  be  avoided  if  the  reader  keeps  in 
mind  the  operational  definition  of  each  index.  It  may  be  helpful  to 
present  a  diagrammatic  operational  definition  of  these  twelve  vari- 
ability indices.  They  are  defined  by  the  amount  of  discrepancy  be- 
tween levels  or  areas  of  personality  as  illustrated  in  Figure  31. 

Several  points  require  comment.  First,  it  will  be  noted  that  the  re- 
lationships of  Level  IV  (the  level  of  the  unexpressed  unconscious)  to 
the  other  levels  are  not  included.  No  data  are  available  for  this  area  of 
personality.  It  must  also  be  noted  that  only  one  circle  is  presented  for 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  253 

each  area  of  "other"  behavior.^  In  practice,  there  are  several  "others" 
who  are  always  included  in  the  personality  diagram.  As  we  recall 
from  the  chapter  on  Level  II  (Chapter  8),  the  conscious  view  of 
mother,  father,  and  spouse  (and,  where  possible,  the  therapist)  is 
routinely  included  in  the  personality  pattern.  Similarly,  in  scoring 
fantasy  material,  we  separate  the  themes  attributed  to  father,  mother, 
and  cross-sex  figures.  These  specialized  circles  are  not  all  included 
in  Figure  31.  We  have  included  an  extra  Level  II  Other  circle,  labeled 
"father,"  to  illustrate  the  variability  indices  of  Familial  equation,  de- 
fined as  the  process  of  consciously  ascribing  similarities  or  differences 
to  various  family  members  or  describing  nonmembers  (such  as  the 
therapist)  as  being  like  or  unlike  family  members.  If  a  patient  de- 
scribes hij  therapist  in  the  same  way  that  he  describes  his  father,  the 
two  indices  will  show  little  or  no  discrepancy.  We  would  be  able  to 
say,  "The  patient  consciously  equates  his  therapist  with  his  father." 
We  have  also  included  an  extra  Level  III  Other  circle  to  illustrate 
the  variability  indices  of  displace?nent,  which  is  defined  as  the  process 
of  consciously  ascribing  to  one  "other"  (e.g.,  father)  the  interpersonal 
traits  which  are  preconsciously  assigned  to  another  "other"  (e.g., 
mother) . 

Operational  Definition  of  Forty-eight  Specific  Variability  hidices 

It  was  mentioned  above  that  the  twelve  generic  variability  indices 
subdivide  into  forty-eight  specific  variability  indices.  If  we  consider 
all  the  permutations  and  combinations  of  interrelatedness  among  the 
levels  and  the  personages  at  each  level,  a  list  of  variability  indices  sev- 
eral times  forty-eight  would  be  obtained.  The  forty-eight  indices  now 
to  be  defined  were  selected  on  the  basis  of  the  theoretical  and  clinical 
meaningfulness.  The  plan  of  exposition  is  as  follows:  We  shall  first 
present  an  operational  definition  for  each  of  the  forty-eight  variability 
indices  and  a  formal  title  for  the  high  and  a  low  discrepancy  for  each. 

The  listing,  coding,  formal  designation,  and  operational  definition  of 
each  variability  index  is  contained  in  Table  1 1 ,  The  key  to  the  num- 
bers and  letters  employed  in  coding  the  variabihty  indices  is  presented 
in  Table  12. 

The  Coding  of  the  Variability  Indices 

The  first  column  in  Table  1 1  gives  the  code  number  of  the  variabil- 
ity index.  The  code  number  is  a  simple,  straightforward  notational 
device  which  summarizes  exactly  what  discrepancy  is  involved  in  this 
index.    Every  code  number  for  a  variability  index  comprises  four 

^  An  exception  to  this  statement— two  circles  are  included  in  Figure  31  for  Level  II 
Other  and  Level  III  Other  to  illustrate  the  indices  of  equation  and  displacement. 


254 


THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


TABLE  11 
Operational  Definition  of  Forty-eight  Indices  of  Variation 


Low   Discrepancy 

High  Discrepancy 

Between  the  Two 

Between  the  Two 

Code 

Measures  Is  Called: 

Measures  Is  Called: 

11  SO 

Role  coincidence 

Role  reciprocity 

12  SS 

Self-perception 

Self-deception 

12  GO 

Other-perception 

Other  misperception 

22  SM 

Conscious  identification 

Conscious  disiden- 

(maternal) 

tification    (maternal) 

22  SF 

Conscious  identification 

Conscious  disiden- 

(paternal) 

tification   (paternal) 

22SSp 

Conscious  identification 

Conscious  disiden- 

(spouse) 

tification    (spouse) 

22  ST 

Conscious  identification 

Conscious   disidentifi- 

with  therapist 

cation  with  therapist 

22  MT 

Maternal-therapist 

Maternal-therapist 

equation 

disequation 

22  FT 

Paternal-therapist 

Paternal-therapis: 

equation 

disequation 

22SpT 

Spouse-therapist 

Spouse-therapist 

equation 

disequation 

22  MF 

Maternal-paternal 

Maternal-paternal 

equation 

disequation 

22MSp 

Maternal-spouse 

Maternal-spouse 

equation 

disequation 

22FSp 

Paternal-spouse 

Paternal-spouse 

equation 

disequation 

23  SH 

"Preconscious" 

"Preconscious" 

duplication 

repression 

23  MH 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (maternal) 

cation  (maternal) 

23  FH 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (paternal) 

cation    (paternal) 

23SpH 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (spouse) 

cation  (spouse) 

23  TH 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion    (therapist) 

cation  (therapist) 

23  SO 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (other) 

cation  (other) 

23  SM 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (maternal) 

cation  (maternal) 

23  SF 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion   (paternal) 

cation    (paternal) 

23SSp 

Cross-level  identifica- 

Cross-level disidentifi- 

tion  (cross-sex) 

cation  (cross-sex) 

23  MM 

Fusion  (maternal) 

Diffusion  (maternal) 

This  Variability  Index 
Is  Operationally  De- 
fined by  the  Discrep- 
ancy Between: 

Level  I  self  vs.  Level  I 

other   (specialized   or 

total) 
Level  I  self  vs. 

Level  II  self 
Level  I  other  vs. 

Level  II  other 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  II  mother 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  II  father 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  II  spouse 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  II  therapist 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  II  therapist 
Level  II  father  vs. 

Level  II  therapist 
Level  II  spouse  vs. 

Level  II   therapist 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  II  father 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  II  spouse 
Level  II  father  vs. 

Level  II  spouse 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  III  hero 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  III  hero 
Level  II  father  vs. 

Level  III  hero 
Level  II  spouse  vs. 

Level  III  hero 
Level  II  therapist  vs. 

Level  III  hero 
Level  II  self  vs. 

Level  III  other 
Level  II  self  vs.  Level 

III  maternal  images 
Level  II  self  vs.  Level 

III  paternal  image 
Level  II  self  vs.  Level 

III  cross-sex  images 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  III  maternal 

image 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY 


^55 


TABLE  11— Continued 
Operational  Definition  of  Forty -eight  Indices  of  Variation 


Code 

Low   Discrepancy 
Between  the  Two 
Measures  Is  Called: 

23  FF 

Fusion    (paternal) 

23  SpSp 

Fusion   (cross-sex) 

23  MF 

Displacement 

23  FM 

Displacement 

23SpM 

Displacement 

23SpF 

Displacement 

23MSp 

Displacement 

23FSp 

Displacement 

23TM 

Displacement 

23  TF 

Displacement 

23TSp        Displacement 


33  HM 

33  HF 
33HSp 
33  HO 
25  IS 
25  IM 
25  IF 
25lSp 
25  IT 


"Preconscious"  identi- 
fication   (maternal) 

"Preconscious"  identi- 
fication (paternal) 

"Preconscious"  identi- 
fication  (cross-sex) 

"Preconscious"  identi- 
fication  (total) 

Self-acceptance 

Maternal  idealization 
Paternal  idealization 
Spouse  idealization 
Therapist  idealization 


High  Discrepancy 
Between  the  Two 
Measures  Is  Called: 

Diffusion  (paternal) 
Diffusion  (cross-sex) 
Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 
Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

Cross-level  diffusion 

"Preconscious"  disiden- 
tification   (maternal) 

"Preconscious"  disiden- 
tification  (paternal) 

"Preconscious"  disiden- 
tification   (cross-sex) 

"Preconscious"  disiden- 
tification   (total) 

Self -rejection 

Maternal  devaluation 
Paternal  devaluation 
Spouse  devaluation 
Therapist  devaluation 


This  Variability  Index 
Is  Operationally  De- 
fined by  the  Discrep- 
ancy Between: 

Level  II  father  vs.  Level 

III  paternal  image 
Level  II  spouse  vs.  Level 

III  cross-sex  image 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  III  paternal 

image 
Level  II  father  vs.  Level 

III  maternal  image 
Level  II  spouse  vs. 

Level  III  maternal 

image 
Level  II  spouse  vs. 

Level  III  paternal 

image 
Level  II  mother  vs. 

Level  III  cross-sex 

image 
Level  II  father  vs. 

Level  III  cross-sex 

image 
Level  II  therapist  vs. 

Level  III  maternal 

image 
Level  II  therapist  vs. 

Level   III   paternal 

image 
Level  II  therapist  vs. 

Level  III  cross-sex 

image 
Level  III  hero  vs. 

Level  III  maternal 

image 
Level  III  hero  vs.  Level 

III  paternal  image 
Level  III  hero  vs.  Level 

III  cross-sex  image 
Level  III  hero  vs.  Level 

III  total  other 
Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Level  II  self 
Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Level  II  mother 
Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Level  II  father 
Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Level  II  spouse 
Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Level  II  therapist 


2j6 


THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 


TABLE  11-Continued 
Operational  Definition  of  Forty-eight  Indices  of  Variation 


Code 


35  IH 


35  IM 
35  IF 
35ISp 
35  lO 


Low  Discrepancy 
Between  the  Two 
Measures  Is  Called: 

"Preconscious"  hero 
idealization 

"Preconscious"  mater- 
nal idealization 

"Preconscious"  paternal 
idealization 

"Preconscious"  cross- 
sex  idealization 

"Preconscious"  other 
idealization 


High  Discrepancy 
Between  the  Two 
Measures  Is  Called: 

"Preconscious"   hero 
devaluation 

"Preconscious"    mater- 
nal devaluation 

"Preconscious"  paternal 
devaluation 

"Preconscious"  cross- 
sex  devaluation 

"Preconscious"   other 
devaluation 


This  Variability  Index 
Is  Operationally  De- 
fined by  the  Discrep- 
ancy Between: 

Level  V  ideal  vs. 

Ill  hero 
Level  V  ideal  vs.  Level 

III  maternal  images 
Level  V  ideal  vs.  Level 

III  paternal   images 
Level  V  ideal  vs.  Level 

III  cross-sex  images 
Level  V  ideal  vs.  Level 

III  other 


TABLE  12 

Key  to  Numbers  and  Letters  Employed  in  Coding  Variability  Indices 

Number  Codes  Letter  Codes 

1  =  Level  I  S    =  Self  (i.e.  the  subject) 

2  =  Level  II  M  =  Mother  (the  subject's  own  mother 

3  =  Level  III  is  at  Level  II  and  the  maternal  im- 
5  =  Level  V  age  at  Level  III) 

F   =  Father  or  paternal  image 

Sp  =  Spouse  (if  at  Level 'II)  or  cross-sex 

figures  (in  Level  III  fantasies) 
T  =  Therapist 
I    =  Ego  ideal  from  Level  V 


elements:  two  arable  numbers  and  two  letters.  The  numbers  indicate 
which  levels  are  being  compared.  Thus,  "12"  means  that  behavior  at 
Level  I  is  being  compared  with  behavior  at  Level  II;  "22"  indicates 
that  the  comparison  is  between  two  different  scores  at  Level  II.  The 
two  letters  refer  to  the  respective  personages  at  each  level  that  are 
being  compared.  Thus,  "12  SS"  indicates  that  the  subject's  own  be- 
havior seen  by  others  (Level  I)  and  that  seen  by  self  (Level  II)  are 
being  compared.  The  coding  "23  FM"  indicates  that  the  subject's 
conscious  description  (Level  II)  of  his  father  is  being  compared  with 
the  summed  maternal  images  for  Level  III. 

It  will  be  noted  that  any  code  number  is  actually  a  formula  sum- 
mary of  the  processes  involved  in  obtaining  the  index  and  is  thus  an 
abbreviated  operational  definition  of  the  index.  Since  dozens  of  varia- 
bility indices  are  obtained  for  each  patient  studied  in  the  Kaiser  Foun- 
dation project,  the  codings  allow  a  numerical  filing  system  for  variabil- 
ity data. 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY 


57 


The  Titles  of  Variability  Indices 

For  each  variability  index,  there  are  two  titles  which  refer  to  high 
or  low  discrepancies  between  the  two  levels  or  personages  involved. 
If  a  patient's  self-description  is  close  to  his  observed  behavior  (i.e.,  a 
low  discrepancy)  the  first,  or  positive,  designation  "self-perception"  is 
employed.  If  the  two  levels  are  far  apart  (i.e.,  a  high  discrepancy) 
then  the  second,  or  negative,  designation  "self-deception"  is  used. 

The  specific  procedure  for  measuring  the  variability  indices  will  be 
presented  below.  It  will  suffice  here  to  say  that  if  a  discrepancy  be- 
tween two  levels  is  below  the  mean  of  the  normative  sample,  the 
positive  term  (second  column  in  Table  11)  is  assigned.  If  the  dis- 
crepancy is  above  the  mean,  the  negative  term  (third  column)  is  em- 
ployed. 

The  Operational  Definitions  of  Variability  Indices 

The  fourth  column  in  Table  1 1  contains  the  operational  definition 
of  each  of  the  forty-eight  variability  indices.  This  involves  simply  the 
specific  designation  of  the  levels  and  personages  being  compared. 

Methodology  for  Measuring  Variability  Indices 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  psychology  research  project  has  devoted 
several  years  to  the  development  of  methods  for  measuring  the  kind 
and  amount  of  variability  between  levels  of  personality  and  the  kind 
and  amount  of  variability  between  two  tests  of  the  same  level  ad- 
ministered at  different  times.  The  former  are  structural  variability 
indices;  the  latter  are  called  ternporal  variability  indices. 

A  main  criterion  for  an  effective  discrepancy  measure  is  the  re- 
flection of  changes  in  line  with  the  general  meaning  of  the  inter- 
personal circle.  Thus  a  large  numerical  discrepancy  between  two 
levels  or  between  the  same  level  tapped  in  pretherapy  and  posttherapy 
tests  should  designate  an  extreme  change  in  interpersonal  behavior, 
e.g.,  from  submission  to  dominance. 

One  method  of  assessing  discrepancy  involved  measuring  the  linear 
distance  between  the  two  scores  in  centimeters.  This  has  the  ad- 
vantage of  directness  and  simplicity.  It  had  the  overweighing  disad- 
vantage of  doing  violence  to  the  concept  of  the  circle.  A  large  centi- 
meter difference  was  deceptive  where  the  pre-  and  posttherapy  scores 
were  far  from  the  center  of  the  circle.  Both  scores  could  be  in  the 
same  octant  and  involve  a  similar  extreme  interpersonal  behavior  (e.g., 
sadism)  for  which  the  centimeter  distance  index  would  be  very  large. 

The  development  of  the  numerical  diagnostic  codes  (see  Chapter 
12)  made  possible  improved  methods  of  measuring  change.^  In  the  nu- 

*  The  remainder  of  this  chapter  was  written  by  Joan  S.  LaForge. 


2j8  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 

merical  code  system  every  score  falling  in  the  same  octant  is  assigned 
the  same  code  category.  Thus  a  simple  subtraction  process  yields  a 
discrepancy  estimate.  If  the  patient  is  a  5  before  therapy  and  a  2  after 
therapy,  he  has  changed  three  units  (i.e.,  5  —  2  =  3).  At  first  a  crude, 
intuitive  discrepancy  system  was  established.  Arbitrary  values  were 
assigned  to  the  various  differences.  The  comparison  between  the  ex- 
treme and  the  moderate  scores  (e.g.,  74)  became  an  insoluble  problem 
because  there  was  no  provision  for  assigning  discriminatory  weights. 

DOM 


BLACK 

8     7    RED 


Figure  32.    Model  Employed  To  Determine  Summary  Points  on  the  Diagnostic 
Grid  and  To  Calculate  Horizontal  and  Vertical  Discrepancy  Values. 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY 


259 


The  attempt  was  then  made  to  estabHsh  a  set  of  points  on  the  circle 
to  represent  the  eight  octants  at  two  intensity  levels,  a  total  of  sixteen 
points.  Any  point  falling  in  an  octant  is  then  considered  to  fall  always 
at  one  point.  A  model  establishing  these  points  was  derived  in  the  fol- 
lowing way.  The  plane  was  divided  into  two  areas  (inside  and  outside 
areas),  one  standard  deviation  from  the  center  point  being  the  di- 
viding line.  Those  points  in  the  inside  area  were  considered  to  be  of 
moderate  intensity.  The  center  of  mass  of  each  pie-shaped  area  was 
taken  as  the  location  of  the  representative  point  and  derived  from  the 
formula 

where  x  and  y  are  points  along  the  abscissa  and  ordinate,  and  s  is  the 
area,  integrated  over  the  region  R.  Once  this  collection  of  eight  points 
was  established,  the  problem  of  finding  a  point-representation  for  the 
extreme  intensity  of  each  octant  was  a  little  more  difficult.  There  can 
be  no  center  of  mass  because  each  outer  area  is  infinite. 

At  this  juncture,  consideration  of  the  meaning  of  relative  discrep- 
ancies was  taken  into  account.  Clearly  a  change  in  the  same  octant 
should  be  less  than  the  change,  even  at  the  least  intensity,  between  two 
octants.  With  this  principle  in  mind,  a  set  of  points  was  arbitrarily 
selected  to  represent  the  extreme  intensities  for  the  eight  octants,  main- 
taining, for  example,  that  a  red  1 -black  1  discrepancy  be  a  little  less 
than  a  black  i-black  2  discrepancy. 

The  intersection  of  each  point  with  the  x  (hostility-affiliation)  and 
y  (dominance-submission)  axes  was  established,  and  the  continuum  of 
these  points  was  assigned  the  values  from  -\-56  to  —56,  with  zero  at 
the  center  of  the  circle  (Table  11).  Now  it  was  possible  to  establish 
vertical  and  horizontal  components  of  each  discrepancy.  The  geo- 
metric distance  given  by  the  formula 

VdJTl? 

(where  d^  is  the  vertical  discrepancy  and  dv  the  horizontal)  is  then 
taken  as  the  measure  of  discrepancy. 

Here  another  conception  of  the  meaning  of  discrepancies  was  con- 
sidered. At  all  times  the  discrepancy  between  any  two  equally  distant 
points  should  be  the  same,  regardless  of  the  position  of  the  points  on 
the  circle,  i.e.,  red  1-black  1  should  equal  red  2-black  2.  However, 
from  Table  13,  the  following  is  noted:  for  red  1-black  1  the  x  and  y 
discrepancy  components  are  —23  and  —5,  and  for  red  2-black  2  they 
are  —19  and  +13.  The  squares  of  each  respective  discrepancy  are  554 
and  530. 


i6o  THE  VARIABILITY  OF  PERSONALITY 

TABLE  13 
Horizontal  (Lev)  and  Vertical  (Dom)  Values  for  Each  Octant 


Red     I  +56  +11  Black 


Doni 

Lov 

+56 

+  11 

+47 

-32 

+  11 

-56 

-32 

-47 

-S6 

-11 

6 

-47 

+32 

7 

-11 

+  56 

8 

+  32 

+47 

Do?/i 

Lov 

1 

+  33 

+  6 

2 

+28 

-19 

3 

+  6 

-33 

4 

-19 

-28 

5 

-33 

-  6 

6 

-28 

+  19 

7 

-  6 

+  33 

8 

+  19 

+28 

Theoretically,  the  square  roots  of  these  numbers  should  be  equal  if 
our  principle  is  to  be  met.  The  two  square  roots  are  23.537  and  23.022. 
In  all  such  cases  encountered,  the  closest  luhole  number  to  both  square 
roots  was  taken  to  be  the  discrepancy  (in  this  case,  23).  In  this  way 
fourteen  possible  discrepancies  are  obtained.  They  are  presented  in 
Table  14. 

TABLE  14 
All  Possible  Discrlpancies  Around  the  Pair  1-7  and  Their  Magnitudes 


Pair 

Discrepancy 

1   1   (/  /) 

00 

1  /   (i   1) 

23 

7  2 

26 

1  2  (7  2) 

41 

1   2 

44 

7  3 

48 

1  4 

62 

1  5  (7  3) 

66 

7  5 

68 

1  3 

81 

1  4  (7  4) 

84 

1  5  (7  5) 

91 

1  4 

105 

1  5 

114 

Key:  The  italic  numbers  refer  to  moderate  (black)  diagnostic  intensities  and  the 
ronian-face  numerals  lo  exireme  (red)  intensities 

The  model  was  then  examined  in  terms  of  meaning  for  these  dis- 
crepancies and  the  conceptual-numerical  relationships.  Table  14  shows 
one  set  of  discrepancies  and  their  jelative  magnitudes. 

Careful  examination  of  this  table  shows  the  inner  relationships  of  all 
possible  distances  from  one  octant.  All  other  octants  show  the  same 
relationships  because  of  the  equivalent-distant  principle  used  in  estab- 
lishing discrepancy  magnitudes.  Considering  the  extreme  combina- 
tions only,  we  have  the  following  set  of  discrepancies:    1    1  =  00, 


THE  INDICES  OF  VARIABILITY  261 

1  2  =  44,  1  3  =  81,  1  4  =  105,  I  5  =  114.  The  same  grouping  for 
lesser  intensity  combinations  yields  the  following:  1  1  =  00,  1  2  =  26, 
13^  48, 1  4  =  62, 1  S  ^=  68.  Pairwise  comparison  of  equal  octant  but 
different  intensity  groups  shows  that  as  the  distance  around  the  circle 
becomes  greater  the  ratio  of  the  two  discrepancies  becomes  less,  i.e., 
00:00,  26:44,  48:81,  62:105,  68:114.  This  relationship  leads  to  the 
fact  that  the  discrepancy  between  1  5  (most  extreme  of  the  lesser  in- 
tensities) is  between  the  discrepancy  for  13  and  12,  placing  a  greater 
discrepancy  value  for  a  moderately  distant  discrepancy  of  extreme  in- 
tensity, i.e.,  13,  than  on- an  extremely  distant  discrepancy  of  moderate 
intensity,  i.e.,  1  5. 

Placing  all  discrepancies  in  intervals  of  20,  we  have  the  results  in 
Table  15. 

TABLE  15 

Illustration  of  the  Grouping  of  All  Possible  DiscREPANaES 
Involving  the  Diagnostic  Codes  1  and  1 

Code  Discrepancy  Numerical  Value 

1  I  a  1)  00-20 

1  i   (i  1),  1  2  21-40 

1  2  (i  2)   1  2,  /  5  41H50 

1  4,  \  3  {1  I),  1  5  61-80 

I  3,  I  -^  (/  4),  1  5  (i  5)                                                     81-100 

1  4,  1  5  101-120 

This  grouping  shows  approximate  equations  of  the  various  mixed 
discrepancies,  such  2.s  1  4  approximately  equals  1  3  approximately 
equals  1  5,  i.e.,  the  two  most  extreme  moderate-intensity  discrepancies 
are  approximately  equal  to  the  discrepancy  between  moderate  and 
intense  of  medium  distance  around  the  circle. 

A  table  of  weighted  scores  for  each  possible  interlevel  discrepancy 
is  presented  in  Appendix  5. 

This  consideration  of  discrepancy  relationships  seems  to  indicate 
that  the  model  we  constructed  is  consistent  with  the  meaning  of 
change  in  terms  of  the  theory  of  the  interpersonal  circle. 


IV 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Personality 


14 


Theory  of  Multilevel  Diagnosis 


The  preceding  thirteen  chapters  have  presented  a  theory  and  an  em- 
pirical system  of  personaHty.  A  compHcated  array  of  variables  have 
been  described,  and  the  relationships  among  variables  have  been  classi- 
fied. 

In  this  fourth  section  of  the  book  we  are  going  to  apply  this  system 
of  personality  to  the  task  of  clinical  diagnosis  and  prognosis.  The 
numerical  code  diagnosis,  it  will  be  recalled,  provides  65,536  personal- 
ity types — at  four  layers  of  personality.  It  is  clearly  impossible  to  ex- 
pect to  locate  sample  cases  illustrating  each  of  these  65,51)6  multilevel 
combinations.  It  is  equally  out  of  the  question  to  give  a  clinical  de- 
scription of  each  of  these  types. 

To  use  this  diagnostic  system  in  clinical  situations  it  is  necessary 
only  to  employ  the  notion  of  multilevel  analysis  and  to  apply  a  com- 
mon-sense interpretation  of  the  numerical  diagnostic  formula.  The 
eight-digit  diagnostic  code,  it  will  be  recalled,  is  nothing  more  than  a 
shorthand  summary  of  the  way  in  which  the  patient  responded  at  the 
several  levels  of  personality. 

The  system  is  quite  complex  in  the  sense  that  it  provides  for  a  great 
variety  of  types.  But  the  processing  of  the  data  and  the  derivation  of 
the  diagnostic  code  is  a  straightforward  clerical,  technical  (nonpro- 
fessional) task.  The  interpretation  of  the  diagnostic  code  is  not  a  de- 
manding assignment  since  the  diagnosis  for  every  level  simply  denotes 
which  interpersonal  behaviors  the  patient  manifested.  The  chnical 
implications  follow  quite  naturally.  We  simply  ask  the  questions: 
What  does  it  mean  if  he  says  this  about  himself  but  acts  that  way? 
What  does  it  mean  if  he  says  this  and  manifests  these  underlying  be- 
haviors.^ 

Application  of  the  system  is  facilitated  by  some  clinical  experience 
with  it,  and  by  some  knowledge  of  the  empirical  results  obtained  in 
normative  studies.  These  will  be  presented  in  the  eight  clinical  chap- 
ters to  follow. 

265 


2  66  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

There  is,  however,  no  high-powered  theory  which  has  to  be  mas- 
tered. There  is  the  one  assumption  that  all  interpersonal  behavior 
serves  to  reduce  anxiety  and  to  maintain  self-esteem.  The  rest  is  based 
on  behavior.  What  did  the  patient  do,  say,  indirectly  express?  A 
multilevel  summary  of  interpersonal  behavior  yields  considerable  addi- 
tional infoi:mation  about  the  rigidity  of  security  operations  (kind  and 
degree)  or  about  conflicts  and  ambivalences  (kind  and  degree). 

This  information  is  then  used  to  answer  functional  questions  about 
motivation  and  treatment. 

The  system  can  be  seen  as  a  hierarchical  pattern  of  levels  which  un- 
fold symmetrically.  For  research  or  clinical  categorization  it  is  con- 
venient to  work  from  the  surface  into  the  indirect  or  deeper  areas  of 
personality. 

Single-Level  Diagnosis 

In  considering  an  individual  case  or  a  general  research  problem  we 
look  first  at  Level  L  There  are  sixteen  interpersonal  types  (eight  mod- 
erate and  eight  intense)  at  this  overt  behavioral  level.  Much  of  our 
research  has  taken  place  at  this  single  level.  We  have  attempted  to 
discover  what  probability  indices  hold  for  this  level.  We  discover,  for 
example,  that  patients  who  are  hypernormal  (code  8)  at  Level  I  re- 
main in  psychotherapy  only  half  as  long  as  distrustful  (code  4)  pa- 
tients; and  that  ulcer  patients  do  not  differ  significantly  from  hyper- 
tensive patients  at  this  level. 

Double-Level  Diagnosis 

Adding  the  Level  II  material  we  get  a  much  more  complicated  two- 
layer  pattern.  First,  it  should  be  noted  that  there  are  probabiUty  find- 
ings which  allow  us  to  predict  on  the  basis  of  Level  II  alone.  When 
we  combine  the  Level  I  and  II  indices,  new  meaning  appears.  The 
number  of  possible  types  multiplies.  There  are  256  two-level  types 
(16  at  Level  I  X  16  at  Level  II). 

A  double-level  diagnosis  is  useful  because  it  points  up  conflict  or 
discrepancy  in  the  presenting  fa9ade.  Some  patients  give  a  dependent, 
fearful  symptomatic  picture  (Level  1  =  6)  and  may  see  themselves  as 
independent  and  self-confident  (Level  II  =  2).  The  code  label  "62" 
thus  becomes  loaded  with  meaning.  It  points  to  an  ambivalent  moti- 
vation, to  a  marked  misperception  by  the  patient  of  the  eff^ect  of  his 
symptom.  It  complicates  the  clinical  predictions  we  are  to  make  about 
the  patient  since  his  symptoms  (6)  are  dependent,  and  his  self-regard 
is  the  opposite  (2), 

The  double-level  diagnosis  ''66"  forecasts  an  entirely  diff^erent  clini- 
cal course.  Here,  the  docile,  fearful  overt  symptomology  is  duplicated 


THEORY  OF  A1ULTILEVEL  DIAGNOSIS  267 

by  the  conscious  self -perception.    A  two-layer  commitment  to  the 
same  interpersonal  operations  is  indicated. 

Triple-Level  Diagnosis 

The  summary  code  of  the  subject's  fantasy-hero  behavior  provides 
the  third  digit  for  the  diagnostic  formula.  There  are  sixteen  fantasy- 
hero  codes  (eight  moderate  and  eight  extreme).  When  these  are  com- 
bined with  the  double-level  codes,  a  total  of  4,096  diagnostic  types  ex- 
ists (256  double-level  types  X  16). 

When  we  consider  the  third  digit  in  any  diagnostic  formula,  con- 
siderable empirical  information  is  available.  We  know,  for  example, 
that  the  fantasy-hero  score  predicts  future  behavior.  Thus,  the  third 
digit  is  of  clinical  interest  in  itself.  It  indicates  what  shifts  in  con- 
scious self-perception  we  can  anticipate.  When  combined  with  the 
first  digits  it  fills  out  a  more  meaningful  pattern. 

A  "773,"  for  example,  denotes  a  patient  who  is  friendly  and  over- 
conventional  at  the  levels  of  overt  presentation  and  conscious  self- 
description.  The  third  digit,  "3,"  indicates  a  "preconscious"  concern 
with  hostility.  It  suggests  that  underlying  antisocial  feeUngs  exist  be- 
neath a  fa9ade  of  bland  normahty  and  that  they  will  probably  appear 
in  future  behavior  at  the  overt  levels. 

The  clinical  meaning  of  a  "773"  is  very  different  from  a  "777." 
The  latter  maintains  a  solid,  triple-layer  structure  of  affiliative,  over- 
conventionality.  Self-satisfaction  would  probably  be  high  and  moti- 
vation for  therapy  low,  since  the  patient  cannot  tolerate  hostile  or  un- 
conventional feelings  at  any  of  the  top  three  layers.  A  "773"  would  be 
handled  quite  differently,  clinically,  since  a  conflict  exists  between  a 
conventional  fa9ade  and  underlying  "preconscious"  sadistic  feelings. 

Four-Level  Diagnosis 

The  fourth  digit  in  the  diagnostic  code  denotes  the  themes  at- 
tributed to  fantasy  "others."  There  is  less  empirical  significance  or 
clinical  meaning  attached  to  this  layer.  This  level  has  not  been  studied 
extensively,  and  no  specific  empirical  significance  attaches  to  it.  This 
layer  does  suggest  how  rigid  or  flexible  the  subject's  range  of  security 
operations  is.  If  themes  which  are  avoided  at  the  top  three  layers  were 
to  appear  in  the  fourth  code  digit,  then  we  might  assume  that  the 
subject  does  not  completely  avoid  that  area.  Consider,  for  example, 
two  patients  who  present  triple-layer  structures  of  solid  distrust  and 
bitterness  (444) .  One  might  have  a  fourth  digit  of  "4,"  which  would 
indicate  a  complete  commitment  to  schizoid  operations.  The  second 
patient  might  present  an  "8"  in  his  "preconscious-other."  The  code 
"4448"  indicates  that  some  tender,  responsible  feelings  exist  and  can 


268  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

be  tolerated,  at  least  at  this  more  indirect  level  of  expression,  A  com- 
mon-sense hypothesis  might  be  that  the  latter  patient  would  have  a 
slightly  less  pessimistic  prognosis  than  the  patient  who  could  not  al- 
low any  affiliative  behavior  at  any  level. 

The  fourth  digit  is,  therefore,  included  in  the  diagnostic  code  but 
is  given  minimal  consideration  in  the  clinical  sections  to  follow. 

When  the  16  "preconscious-other"  codes  are  combined  with  the 
4,096  triple-layer  types,  a  total  of  65,536  is  obtained. 

Organization  of  the  Interpersonal  Typology 

A  system  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  which  involves  this  many  types 
may  appear  bewildering  in  its  scope.  We  have  stressed,  however,  that 
the  system  is  fairly  simple  to  apply  if  the  common-sense  meaning  of 
any  particular  multilevel  combination  is  kept  in  mind.  First  the  eight- 
digit  formula  is  derived  for  a  patient.  To  understand  the  patient's  per- 
sonality organization  we  simply  translate  the  code  digits  into  diagnos- 
tic terms.  The  conflicts  or  rigid  duplications  existing  in  the  multilevel 
pattern  will  become  apparent. 

The  eight  diagnostic  chapters  which  follow  present  the  clinical  and 
research  data  now  available.  These  chapters  refer  to  the  eight  typo- 
logical categories  at  Levels  I  and  IL  The  "schizoid"  chapter  is  con- 
cerned with  patients  who  present  as  "44's"  at  Levels  I  and  II.  In 
each  clinical  chapter  the  general  findings  typical  of  the  pure,  uncon- 
flicted  case  will  be  presented. 


15 

Adjustment  Through  Rebellion: 
The  Distrustful  Personality ' 


This  chapter  deals  with  those  individuals  who  select  distrust  and  re- 
bellion as  their  solutions  to  life's  problems.  This  is  the  "44"  personal- 
ity type.  In  their  crucial  relationships  with  others,  these  human  beings 
consistently  maintain  attitudes  of  resentment  and  deprivation.  They 
handle  anxiety  by  establishing  distance  between  themselves  and  others. 
At  the  critical  moments  of  relationship  with  others  they  become  cyni- 
cal, passively  resistant,  and  bitter. 

The  distrustful  way  of  life  is  in  some  ways  a  puzzling  phenomenon. 
The  ideals  of  our  culture  stress  adjustment,  closeness,  and  cooperation. 
It  is  generally  taken  for  granted  that  trustful,  loving  relations  with  cer- 
tain important  others  is  one  of  the  basic  human  goals.  There  exists, 
however,  a  very  large  group  of  individuals  who  consistently  avoid 
this  relationship.  They  compulsively  eschew  closeness  with  others. 
They  are  traumatized  and  threatened  by  positive  feelings. 

These  human  beings  often  do  not  voluntarily  seek  distance  and  dis- 
appointment from  others.  In  their  conscious  ideals,  on  the  contrary, 
they  may  strive  and  long  for  tenderness.  They  are  usually  frustrated, 
depressed,  and  most  dissatisfied  with  their  situations. 

They  regularly  manifest,  however,  the  reflexes  of  distrust  and  re- 
sentment. They  involuntarily  provoke  rejection  and  punishment  from 
others.  They  cannot  tolerate  durable  relationships  of  conformity  or 
collaboration. 

The  Purpose  of  Distrustful  Behavior 

Those  human  beings  who  are  overtly  bitter  and  cynical  have  se- 
lected these  operations  because  they  find  them  most  effective  in  ward- 

*  In  this  chapter  and  the  subsequent  seven,  we  shall  be  discussing  pure  interpersonal 
types  based  on  Level  I-M  and  II-C  diagnosis.  We  shall  describe  the  unconflicted  sub- 
ject who  presents  the  same  security  operations  in  his  symptomatic  behavior  and  in  his 
conscious  self-descriptions.  Space  does  not  permit  a  consideration  of  the  conflicted 
types. 

269 


270  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

ing  off  anxiety.  Pain  and  discomfort  are  traditionally  associated  with 
alienation  from  others,  but  for  these  subjects  this  discomfort  is  less 
than  the  anxiety  involved  in  trustful,  tender  feehngs.  For  the  person 
who  has  experienced  past  rejections  or  humiliations  there  are  certain 
comforts  and  rewards  in  developing  a  rebellious  protection.  The 
essence  of  this  security  operation  is  a  malevolent  rejection  of  con- 
ventionality. Trust  in  others,  cooperation,  agreeability,  and  affilia- 
tion seem  to  involve  a  certain  loss  of  individuahty.  Giving  or  sharing 
or  trusting  requires  a  sacrifice  of  pure  narcissism  and  some  relinquish- 
ing of  the  critical  function. 

The  rebellious  adjustment  provides  a  feehng  of  difference  and 
uniqueness  which  is  most  rewarding  to  some  individuals.  Inevitable 
ties  and  responsibilities  go  with  an  agreeable,  conventional  adjustment. 
For  the  person  who  avoids  this  way  of  life  there  are  certain  rewards 
— a  rebellious  freedom,  a  retaliatory  pleasure  in  rejecting  the  conven- 
tional, a  delight  in  challenging  the  taboos,  commitments,  and  expecta- 
tions which  are  generally  connected  with  a  durable  affiliative  rela- 
tionship. 

In  the  extreme  case,  the  security  opeiations  of  distrustful  aliena- 
tion involve  a  spiteful  and  bitter  rejection  of  love  and  closeness.  This 
phenomenon  has  been  best  understood  by  Sullivan.  He  has  given  a 
most  thoughtful  description  of  this  process: 

Some  years  ago,  the  young  nephew  of  one  of  my  friends  was  admitted  to 
the  Henry  Phipps  Psychiatric  Clinic.  The  patient  was  suffering  an  acute 
schizophrenic  disturbance,  catatonic  in  type.  He  was  placed  under  principal 
care  of  a  close  friend  of  mine,  and  I  followed  developments  closely  and  saw  the 
patient  occasionally.  As  he  became  unmanageable,  he  was  transferred  to  the 
Sheppard  and  Enoch  Pratt  Hospital,  arriving  there  mute  and  requiring  feeding 
by  the  nasal  tube.  He  was  extremely  resistive  to  this  feeding  unless  I  did  it,  in 
which  latter  case  he  came  to  help  with  the  insertion  of  the  tube.  I  thoughtlessly 
took  over  on  all  these  occasions  and  otherwise  greatly  interested  myself  in  him. 
As  he  was  convalescing  quite  nicely,  he  underwent  what  I  call  a  malevolent 
transformation  of  interpersonal  relations  and  became  first  mischievous  and  later 
definitely  "hateful"  on  the  ward.  The  outcome  was  a  chronic  dilapidating  ill- 
ness requiring  State  Hospital  care. 

From  the  few  facts  recited  above  and  sundry  other  observations  in  my  own 
and,  mediately,  other  psychiatrists'  work  I  inferred  the  theory  of  malevolent 
transformation  of  "personality,"  now  taught  in  the  Washington  School  of  Psy- 
chiatry, after  considerable  supporting  evidence  as  to  its  current  adequacy  had 
been  derived  from  data  on  personality  development. 

In  brief,  this  theory  holds  that  if  one  progresses  into  a  relatively  enduring 
situation  in  which  one's  indicated  needs  for  tenderness  are  customarily  re- 
buffed, one  comes  to  manifest  malevolent  behavior  when  one  needs  tenderness, 
in  lieu  of  showing  the  need,  and  to  expect— and  by  this  pattern  all  but  guarantee 
—an  unfavorable  attitude  towards  one  in  others.  (6,  pp.  451-52) 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  271 

The  purpose  of  the  malevolent  transformation,  we  assume,  is  to 
avoid  the  intense  anxiety  created  by  the  patient's  tender  feeHngs. 
These  patients  apparently  have  come  to  expect  that  loving  feelings  in 
themselves  or  in  others  are  the  prelude  to  anxiety  and  rejection.  The 
reflexes  of  bitter  distrust  resolve  this  dilemma  very  nicely.  Such  re- 
flexes w^ard  oflF  one's  own  trustful  feelings  and  tend  to  push  away  the 
other  person. 

In  moderate  intensity  the  "44"  security  operations  of  rebellious 
skepticism  have  certain  adaptive  advantages  for  the  individual  and  for 
society.  They  are  associated  with  a  healthy,  critical  approach  to  the 
accepted  conventions  and  to  the  accepted  forms  of  social  relationship. 
There  is  a  familiar  observation  that  every  creative  expression  is  an  act 
of  rebellion,  a  critical  questioning  of  some  conventional  concept. 
Skepticism  gives  the  human  being  a  sense  of  freedom  and  uniqueness. 
It  protects  against  surprises.  A  mildly  disappointed  cynicism  is  an  ex- 
cellent preparation  for  future  disappointments. 

The  critical,  rebellious  person  can  play  a  most  healthy  role  in  any 
social  group.  Docile  inertia  or  fearful-need-to-conform  or  need-to-be- 
liked  can  lead  to  a  stultifying  atmosphere.  There  are  valuable  rewards 
for  the  successful  rebel  who  maintains  a  realistic,  accurate  skepticism 
toward  the  accepted  ways  of  doing  things. 

James  Joyce  has  provided  an  interesting  illustration  of  this  rela- 
tionship between  bitter  rebellion  and  creativity.  When  his  hero  dedi- 
cates his  Hfe  to  art  he  adopts  the  motto  non  credo,  non  serviam  and 
recognizes  that  this  rejection  of  family,  church,  and  society  commits 
him  to  a  life  of  "silence,  exile  and  cunning." 

The  "44"  mode  of  adjustment  has  been  eulogized  by  many  writers. 
Its  most  enthusiastic  advocate  is  Robert  Lindner.  He  states:  "It  is  pos- 
sible, then,  to  escape  from  history,  to  break  out  of  the  cage  whose 
outer  limits  never  have  worn  smooth  and  deeply  grooved  with  endless 
pacing.  And  it  is  possible  to  do  this  without  the  letting  of  blood,  with- 
out violence,  without  the  sacrifice  of  basic  values.  All  that  is  re- 
quired is  to  reach  for  one  cup  wherein  the  heady  mixture  of  true  re- 
bellion, the  brew  of  sweet  life-affirming  protest,  has  been  poured, 
for  this — and  this  alone —  is  the  elixir  vitae."   (3,  p.  296) 

This  author  has  taken  one  mode  of  adjustment  (at  one  level  of  per- 
sonality) and  has  made  it  the  key  to  mental  health.  In  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  system,  the  overt  reflex  security  operation  of  rebellious 
nonconformity  is  one  of  eight  generic  security  operations,  each  of 
which  has  an  adaptive  and  a  maladaptive  intensity. 

Skeptical  alienation  from  convention  and  from  acceptance  of  others 
can  serve  several  purposes  for  the  individual  who  selects  this  way  of 


272  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

life.  These  include:  protection  for  disappointment,  realistic  critical 
rejection  of  the  conventional,  the  warding  ojff  of  anxiety  generated  by 
trust  and  tenderness,  the  freedom  associated  with  uniqueness  and  re- 
bellious individuality,  and,  in  the  pathological  extreme,  malevolent  re- 
taliation for  the  feelings  of  rejection  by  society  in  general  or  specific 
"other  ones." 

The  Effect  of  Distrustful  Behavior 

Bitter  rebellious  behavior  pulls  punitive  rejection  and  superiority 
from  others.  In  systematic  language,  FG  provokes  BCD;  crime  pro- 
vokes, punishment. 

In  the  passage  just  quoted  Sullivan  has  described  this  phenomenon 
very  clearly.  He  speaks  of  this  pattern  almost  guaranteeing  an  un- 
favorable attitude  in  others.  A  sour,  distrustful  approach  invariably 
establishes  distance  from  others,  provoking  them  to  ignore,  condemn, 
or  disaffiliate. 

In  the  case  of  the  adaptively,  moderately  rebellious  person  the  same 
reaction  develops  to  a  milder  degree.  We  consider  here  the  individual 
who  communicates  in  his  actions,  his  demeanor,  and  his  interpersonal 
reflexes  a  message  of  skepticism  and  passive  rejection  of  conventional- 
ity. These  persons  are  seen  as  iconoclastic,  eccentric,  different,  creat- 
ive. Originality  is  inevitably  linked  to  rebellion,  i.e.,  rejection  of  the 
established,  the  authoritative,  the  conventional.  The  iconoclastic  ap- 
proach usually  pulls  irritated  rejection  from  those  who  represent  au- 
thority and  from  those  who  conform  to  it. 

One  of  the  most  consistent  and  interesting  results  of  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  research  has  been  the  empirical  importance  attached  to 
the  conformity-nonconformity  axis  of  the  interpersonal  diagnostic 
circle.  Conventionality  (as  measured  by  the  points  L,  M,  and  N  on  the 
circle  at  Levels  I  and  II)  is  closely  related  to  absence  of  overt  anxiety, 
to  the  presence  of  psychosomatic  symptoms,  to  a  state  of  low  moti- 
vation for  psychotherapy,  and  to  many  other  personality  variables 
(see  Chapter  18).  The  nonconventional  operations  of  distrust,  rebel- 
lion, and  alienation  are  defined  by  the  opposite  end  of  the  LMN  axis, 
i.e.,  by  the  points  F  and  G  on  the  circle. 

The  individuals  whose  overt  operations  emphasize  nonconformity 
and  skeptical  distrust  invariably  isolate  and  alienate  themselves  from 
others.  Conventional  people  are  often  irritated  and  made  anxious  by 
the  sullen,  rebel.  Even  the  most  agreeable  and  overtly  friendly  souls 
can  be  provoked  to  disapproval  when  faced  with  distrustful  opera- 
tions. 

The  psychotherapy  group  provides  an  excellent  locale  for  observ- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  273 

ing  these  processes.  Group  members  are  quickly  trained  to  reject  or 
isolate  themselves  from  the  sullen  patient.  By  their  tone  of  voice,  their 
gestures,  often  by  their  dress,  these  patients  communicate  the  message, 
"I  am  different;  I  distrust  and  disagree  with  you." 

The  principle  of  reciprocal  relations  operates  in  the  case  of  the  re- 
bellious personality  with  impressive  and  depressing  results.  These  pa- 
tients provoke  disregard  and  hostility  from  others.  This  behavior  on 
the  part  of  others  leads  to  an  increase  in  retaliatory  distrust.  The  sul- 
len, distrustful  person  creates  for  himself  a  world  of  punitive  rejection. 

These  reciprocal  processes  do  not  work  with  uniform  consistency. 
There  are  some  individuals  who  are  so  committed  to  friendly,  nur- 
turant  responses  that  they  do  not  immediately  react  with  hostility 
when  faced  with  distrustful  reflexes  in  another.  They  may  attempt  to 
win  the  sullen  person  over  into  a  close  relationship.  Where  the  rebel- 
lious fagade  is  adaptable  and  not  extreme,  this  may  lead  to  a  relaxation 
of  the  distrustful  defenses.  This  often  happens  in  social  and  thera- 
peutic experiences. 

Where  the  distrustful  reflexes  are  intense  and  are  the  sole  means  of 
warding  off  anxiety,  then  positive  feelings  in  the  "other  one"  tend  to 
be  rebuffed.  This  bitter  reaction  will  eventually  discourage  the  most 
persistently  friendly  "other"  and  will  inevitably  lead  to  irritation. 

The  severely  distrustful  person  is  most  comfortable  when  he  is  ex- 
pressing bitter  feelings.  He  is  threatened  and  suspicious  of  tenderness 
which  can  be  viewed  as  an  intolerable  threat  to  his  mode  of  adjust- 
ment. The  common  assumption  that  what  the  deprived,  distrustful 
person  needs  is  love  and  affection  can  be  seen  to  be  a  well-meaning  but 
naive  notion.  To  the  person  with  a  set  of  severely  crippled  reflexes 
tenderness  in  the  "other  one"  is  a  loaded  gun — a  most  frightening  and 
fearful  stimulus.  The  "malevolent  transformation"  described  by  Sulli- 
van is  often  the  reaction  to  the  threat  of  affection. 

D.  H.  Lawrence  has  provided  us  with  a  clear  illustration  of  the  way 
in  which  the  distrustful,  disaffiliated  person  avoids  tender  feelings. 
The  hero  of  Aaron's  Rod  announces:  "I  don't  want  my  Fate  or  my 
Providence  to  treat  me  well.  I  don't  want  kindness  or  love.  I  don't 
believe  in  harmony  and  people  loving  one  another.  I  believe  in  the 
fight  and  in  nothing  else.  I  believe  in  the  fight  which  is  in  everything. 
And  if  it  is  a  question  of  women,  I  believe  in  the  fight  of  love,  even  if  it 
blinds  me.  And  if  it  is  a  question  of  the  world,  I  believe  in  fighting  it 
and  in  having  it  hate  me,  even  if  it  breaks  my  legs.  I  want  the  world  to 
hate  me,  because  I  can't  bear  the  thought  that  it  might  love  me.  For 
of  all  things  love  is  the  most  deadly  to  me,  and  especially  from  such  a 
repulsive  world  as  I  think  this  is.  .  .  ."  (1,  pp.  307-8) 


2  74  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


Clinical  Manifestation  of  Distrust  and  Rebellion  ^ 

The  symptomatic  correlates  of  this  mode  of  overt  adjustment  are 
quite  typical,  and  clearly  different  from  other  diagnostic  types. 

These  patients  exhibit  sour,  pessimistic,  or  indifferent  feelings.  This 
may  often  appear  to  be  a  fiat  affect  or  an  absence  of  feelings.  This  is 
probably  an  incomplete  and  misguided  interpretation.  There  is  no 
evidence  to  indicate  that  the  distrustful  person  feels  less  intensely.  It 
is  necessary  to  look  at  the  interpersonal  implications  of  a  resigned  or 
skeptical  approach.  These  patients  do  not  admit  to  conventional  reac- 
tions. Their  nonconformist  facade  means  that  they  express  different 
feelings  in  different  ways.  They  are  communicating  by  their  actions 
and  their  verbalizations  an  intense  and  emotionally  loaded  message  of 
sullen  distrust. 

Clinically  this  attitude  may  be  expressed  in  the  generic  motto:  "I  am 
a  sullen,  disappointed  person;  you  can't  do  anything  for  me." 

These  patients  do  not  participate  in  therapeutic  planning  with 
docile  eagerness  or  enthusiastic  hope.  They  may  agree  to  treatment, 
but  the  note  of  skeptical  passive  resistance  is  often  obvious. 

In  regard  to  symptoms,  these  patients  tend  not  to  have  psychoso- 
matic ailments;^  nor  do  they  complain  of  the  overt  anxiety  of  the 
phobic  or  the  worries  of  the  obsessive.  They  present  characterological 
or  straightforward  interpersonal  disorders.  They  tend  to  complain  of 
marital  discord,  social  isolation,  frustration,  distance  and  disappoint- 
ment in  their  relations  with  others.  A  most  typical  symptom  is  occu- 
pational or  academic  difficulty.  They  may  describe  a  history  of  re- 
bellion against  authority,  and  are  often  stalemated  in  their  vocation. 
They  are  frank  to  admit  their  disillusionment  and  irritation  with  others. 
They  tend  to  complain  of  their  treatment  at  the  hands  of  others;  yet, 
in  contrast  to  some  of  the  poignant  masochists  described  in  the  foUow- 

^  In  this  secdon  and  in  the  "Clinical  Manifestation"  sections  of  the  following  seven 
chapters,  we  shall  consider  the  symptomatic  pictures  presented  by  the  various  diag- 
nostic types.  These  discussions  are  highly  generalized  and  suggestive.  Two  qualifica- 
tions must  be  kept  in  mind.  First,  we  are  considering  here  the  symptomatic  picture 
of  the  pure  type  (in  this  chapter  the  "44").  Variations  in  behavior  at  other  levels  can 
change  the  symptomatic  presentation;  thus,  the  "41"  comes  to  the  clinic  presenting  a 
facade  diflferent  from  the  "44."  The  second  qualification  refers  to  the  precipitating 
cause  for  psychiatric  referral.  Most  of  the  patients  coming  to  the  psychiatric  clinic 
are  in  some  state  of  anxiety.  Often  something  has  happened  recently  to  threaten  their 
overt  security  operations  (whether  they  are  schizoid  or  hysterical).  We  are  con- 
sidering, in  this  section,  the  general  clinical  impression  made  by  the  patient  which  is 
often  quite  different  from  the  "current"  anxiety  which  brings  him  to  the  clinic. 

^  In  one  diagnostic  study  comparing  the  Level  I  interpersonal  diagnoses  of  a  group 
of  psychosomaoc  and  neurotic  patients,  only  7  per  cent  of  the  psychosomatics  fell  into 
the  rebellious-distrustful  octant  {FC)  of  the  diagnostic  grid,  whereas  43  per  cent  fell 
into  the  opposite  sector.  (2) 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  275 

ing  chapter,  they  do  not  attempt  to  win  pity  or  to  present  them- 
selves as  good  and  blameless.  They  stress  instead  a  grievance  against 
the  M^orld,  a  pessimistic  disappointment  with  self  and  others. 

These  security  operations,  it  will  be  noted,  do  not  lend  themselves 
to  a  well-motivated,  eager  acceptance  of  psychotherapy.  They  often 
agree  to  treatment  with  a  half-hearted  pessimism:  "I  guess  I'll  have  to; 
I  don't  see  any  other  solution,"  etc.  These  patients  often  express  pas- 
sive complaints  about  the  kind  of  therapy  offered,  about  the  therapist 
to  whom  they  are  assigned,  about  the  necessity  to  be  in  a  clinic,  etc. 
Often  these  patients  will  sullenly  refuse  the  therapy  that  is  recom- 
mended. For  example,  they  may  interpret  the  assignment  to  group 
therapy  as  a  sign  of  rejection  by  the  clinic. 

In  the  case  of  the  moderate  rebel,  these  gloomy,  resistant  operations 
may  not  become  apparent.  They  may  employ  a  sarcastic,  self-immo- 
lating humor.  They  may  describe  their  isolation  and  disappointment 
with  a  bitter,  wry  irony.  If  they  sense  honesty  and  reasonability  in 
the  clinician  they  may  muffle  or  shelve  their  skepticism. 

Regardless  of  the  intensity  or  rigidity  of  the  character  structure, 
there  is  one  interpersonal  rule  which  invariably  holds  for  the  "44" 
personality.  They  are  painfully  sensitive  to  phoniness,  pomposity, 
naive  obtuseness,  or  arrogance  on  the  part  of  the  "other  one."  These 
patients  tend,  as  a  group,  to  load  their  perceptions  of  others  with  a 
hostile  skepticism.  They  look  for  dishonesty  and  hostility  in  others. 
They  are  incredibly  sensitive  instruments  for  picking  up  rejection  or 
punitive  feelings  in  others.  Naive  hysterical  patients,  on  the  contrary, 
tend  to  act  on  the  assumption  that  others  (in  their  in-group)  are  con- 
ventional and  sweet  like  themselves. 

We  have  noted  in  Chapter  7  that  all  maladjusted  persons  are  skilled 
in  provoking  others  to  certain  reciprocal  responses.  The  distrustful 
patient  is  most  accomplished  in  pulling  bureaucratic  or  moral  disap- 
proval from  others.  He  often  puts  the  therapist  to  elaborate  tests 
aimed  at  provoking  impatience  or  moral  censure.  He  compulsively 
clings  to  the  often  automatic  and  involuntary  conviction  that  the 
clinician  fails  to  understand  him,  or  acts  in  a  pompous,  overconven- 
tional  manner.  He  specializes  in  provoking  the  therapist  to  set  limits 
and  re-create  an  authority-rebellion  or  rejecting-distrustful  relation- 
ship. 

The  distrustful  "44"  personality  type  described  in  this  chapter  has 
certain  similarities  to  a  behavior  pattern  observed  in  group  psycho- 
therapy by  Jerome  Frank  et  al.  (5,  pp.  215)  Frank  calls  this  type  the 
"help-rejecting  complainer"  and  states  that  the  pattern  "consists  of  a 
patient's  continuing  attempt  in  the  group  to  elicit  help — often  without 
actually  asking  for  it — and  his  attempt  to  prove  greater  need  than 


276  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

other  people,  while  either  imphcitly  or  explicitly  rejecting  all  help 
offered.  This  pattern  seems  to  be  an  expression  of  conflict  between 
the  patient's  perception  of  himself  as  needing  help  and  his  anger  at 
all  potential  help-givers  for  being  unable  or  unwilling  to  supply  it. 
His  behavior  justifies  his  anger  toward  the  help-givers  and  maintains 
his  claim  for  help  while  preventing  him  from  becoming  dependent 
on  the  distrusted  potential  help-givers." 

We  have  so  far  stressed  the  symptomatic  and  interpersonal  aspects 
of  the  clinical  picture.  There  are  certain  psychometric  correlates  of 
the  rebellious  presentation  which  appear  on  personality  tests — for 
example,  the  MMPI  and  the  Rorschach — which  are  independent  of 
the  interpersonal  system. 

Patients  who  behave  in  a  sullen,  distrustful  manner  (Level  I)  have 
a  typical  pattern  on  the  MMPL  Their  high  peaks  fall  on  depression, 
schizoid,  and  psychopathic  scales.  They  also  have  elevations  on  the  F 
scale  which  is  a  rough  measure  of  nonconformity.  They  generally 
do  not  have  elevations  on  the  L,  K,  Hy,  and  Hs  scales. 

This  suggests  that  pessimistic  dysphoria  (D),  alienation  (Sc),  rebel- 
lious disidentification  (Pd),  and  nonconventionality  (F)  are  character- 
istics of  the  distrustful  personality.  The  scales  on  which  they  show 
low  scores  are  those  related  to  denial  of  antisocial  or  hostile  tendencies 
and  to  a  naive,  conventional,  sweet  fagade.  The  distrustful  personality 
can  be  differentiated  on  the  MMPI  from  the  obsessive-masochist.  The 
latter  have  pronounced  depression  and  psychasthenic  scales.  The 
former  exhibit  schizoid  scores  which  are  higher  than  psychasthenia; 
and,  though  the  depression  scores  are  elevated,  they  are  not  as  marked. 
The  higher  the  F,  the  more  likely  that  rebellion  and  not  masochism  is 
the  security  operation. 

On  the  Rorschach  or  TAT  these  patients  characteristically  manifest 
different,  odd,  idiosyncratic  content.  Unconventional  themes  are 
common — bizarre  situations,  freely  described  sexual  themes,  and  poor 
form  responses. 

Interpersonal  Definition  of  the  Schizoid  Maladjustment 

Chapter  12  presented  evidence  that  certain  standard  psychiatric 
diagnoses  were  related  to  specific  interpersonal  patterns.  Extreme, 
imbalanced  social  patterns  thus  can  help  to  establish  psychiatric  diag- 
nosis. 

Distrustful,  intensely  rebellious  behavior  is  characteristic  of  the 
schizoid  personality.  Such  a  personality  shows  maladjustment  essen- 
tially in  bitter,  disappointed  alienation  and  tends  to  handle  anxiety  by 
avoiding  close,  tender  contacts  with  other  individuals  and  by  avoiding 
close  commitments  to  society  in  general.  In  the  extreme  case  this  be- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  277 

comes  a  malevolent  rejection  of  people  and  of  conventional  social 
standards. 

Many  of  the  symptoms  of  the  schizoid  condition  may  be  inter- 
preted in  the  light  of  interpersonal  communication.  They  seem  to  be 
expressions  of  a  bitter  alienation  from  accepted  standards,  a  refusal  to 
conform  which  in  the  extreme  case  becomes  a  rigid  pathological  in- 
ability to  conform.  Highly  individualistic,  eccentric  behavior  is  (in 
the  absence  of  organic  disease)  generally  pathognomonic  of  schizo- 
phrenia. It  seems  possible  ro  think  of  this  as  being  an  intense,  and  often 
desperate,  attempt  to  express  difference,  to  establish  a  complete,  bit- 
ter break  from  conventional  reality. 

Kobler,  speaking  from  therapeutic  experience  with  schizophrenics 
in  the  Pinel  Foundation  Hospital,  believes  that  schizoid  malevolence 
can  be  seen  as  asking  the  question,  "Even  if  I  do  this  and  am  Uke  this, 
can  you  still  love  me?"  The  therapeutic  staff  at  Pinel  senses  hope  and 
a  violent  testing  of  the  therapist  with  the  anticipation  of,  "No,  the 
other  cannot  love."  When  the  reaction  of  the  therapist  is  not  rejec- 
tion, Kobler  states  that  there  is  further  negativistic  testing  and  at  the 
same  time  the  continued  hope  of  finally  finding  the  one  who  will  not 
reject. 

The  Schizoid  Psychosis 

The  interpersonal  effect  of  bizarre  behavior  is  to  provoke  exas- 
perated rejection  from  others.  Marked  eccentricity  flaunts  to  the 
world  the  message,  "I  do  not  accept  your  ideals  of  conduct;  I  do  not 
conform.  I  do  not  want  your  approval."  This  usually  guarantees  to 
the  subject  the  disapproval  of  others.  In  the  extreme  case  (psychosis) 
it  provokes  society  to  punitive  incarceration. 

The  tendency  for  bizarre  behavior  to  pull  rejection  from  others 
was  illustrated  by  the  reactions  of  some  therapy  group  members  to  the 
schizoid  fantasies  of  a  fellow  member.  This  particular  group  had 
been  meeting  for  almost  a  year  and  an  unusually  frank,  honest  recog- 
nition and  acceptance  of  each  other  had  been  developed.  One  of  the 
members  was  a  chronic,  severe,  ex-state-hospital  schizoid  who  had  ap- 
parently never  been  able  to  integrate  a  friendly,  trusting  relationship 
with  another  human  being.  The  group  had  initially  ignored  and  de- 
spised her.  By  the  fourth  month,  her  ability  to  train  others  to  reject 
her  was  the  focus  of  considerable  study.  Even  after  this  had  been 
worked  through  for  two  more  months,  she  could  be  reduced  to  panic 
by  a  warm  smile  or  casual  friendly  compliment. 

In  one  session  a  woman  who  employed  narcissistic,  exhibitionistic 
operations  described  her  fantasy  of  parading  down  the  street  in  glam- 
orous clothes  in  order  to  provoke  envy  and  admiration  from  others. 


278  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

The  schizoid  woman  then  confided  the  repeated  fantasy  of  run- 
ning naked  out  into  the  street.  The  question  was  then  posed  to  the 
group  what  reaction  would  be  provoked  from  them  at  the  sight  of  a 
neighbor  running  naked  in  the  street.  Their  associations  were,  "I'd 
think  she's  nuts,"  "I'd  call  the  cops  to  come  and  take  her  away,"  etc. 
These  associations  demonstrated  the  effect  of  the  bizarre  fantasy  in 
pulling  rejection  and  intolerant  contempt  from  others.  Expressed  in 
the  context  of  an  honest  and  accepting  group  they  sharpened  the 
schizoid  patient's  understanding  of  the  rebellious  and  alienating  effect 
of  her  security  operations. 

Many  schizoid  or  schizophrenic  symptoms  can  be  interpreted  as 
interpersonal  communications  conveying  to  others  the  theme  of  ma- 
levolent disaffiliation.  The  inability  or  refusal  to  integrate  close  rela- 
tions with  others  and  the  tendency  to  perceive  and  react  differently 
have  such  a  consistent  and  inevitable  impact  on  others  that  they  seem 
to  confirm  the  hypothesis  that  an  interpersonal  purpose  is  involved. 
In  the  extreme  case  these  desperate  violations  of  customs  and  accepted 
social  patterns  become  the  symptoms  of  psychotic  negativism;  autism, 
incontinence,  refusal  to  eat,  etc.  The  bitter,  rebellious  anger  involved 
in  these  behaviors  has  often  been  commented  on  by  clinicians. 

Some  interesting  complications  are  introduced  by  cultural  differ- 
ences. A  schizophrenic  psychosis  is  defined  as  a  desperate,  repetitious, 
malevolent,  distrustful  rebellion.  Now,  the  behaviors  which  express 
these  motives  may  differ  from  one  society  to  another.  Thus,  failure  to 
eat  and  an  insistence  upon  the  reality  of  one's  own  fantasy  life  are,  in 
our  society,  negativistic  and  alienating  behaviors.  In  another  society 
the  same  behavior  may  be  symptomatic  of  an  extreme  desperate  at- 
tempt to  overconform.  It  may  express  the  message  of  frantic  re- 
ligious overconventionality.  The  interpersonal  effect  of  the  symptom 
is  the  key  to  its  diagnostic  meaning. 

A  glance  at  the  symptomatic  signs  of  schizophrenia  listed  in  any 
psychiatric  text  seems  to  suggest  that  most  of  them  are  calculated  to 
provoke  frustration  and  irritation  in  others.  The  symptoms  of  the 
obsessive  state,  by  comparison,  tend  to  provoke  feelings  of  superiority 
in  the  other  one. 

The  rebellious  implication  of  the  schizoid  maladjustment  has  been 
noted  by  other  writers.  Powdermaker  for  example  writes: 

Why  does  the  schizophrenic  use  the  particular  defenses  that  he  does  against 
these  fears  and  conflicts?  Why  does  he  make  himself  ununderstandable  and 
so  different  from  the  social  norm  in  his  relationships,  instead  of  endeavoring  to 
conform  to  the  social  norm  as  the  neurotic  does?  That  the  schizophrenic  is  an 
unsuccessful  rebel  appears  to  be  one  of  the  outstanding  aspects  of  his  behavior. 
This  was  pointed  out  in  the  work  of  Ackerly,  in  which  he  showed  how  the  dc- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  279 

linquent  acts  of  some  of  his  adolescent  patients  had  s^ved  them  from  a  probable 
schizophrenic  breakdown.   (4,  pp.  61-62) 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  Distrustful  Personality 

Here  is  a  summary  of  some  of  the  studies  accomplished  on  the 
schizoid  personality  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  project. 

1.  Patients  who  exhibit  rebellious  distrust  in  their  overt  operations 
do  not  tend  to  have  psychosomatic  symptoms. 

2.  Psychosomatic  patients  do  not  tend  to  utilize  these  interpersonal 
operations  at  Levels  I  or  IL 

3.  Distrust  at  Levels  I  and  II  is  related  to  depression  (D),  noncon- 
formity (F),  schizoid  tendencies  (Sc),  and  rebellious  disidentification 
(Pd)  on  the  MA4PI. 

4.  These  patients  are  among  the  initially  best  motivated  for  psycho- 
therapy. They  do  not  tend  to  terminate  their  clinic  contacts  after 
evaluation  but  are  likely  to  go  on  into  treatment  and  to  remain  in  treat- 
ment. 

5.  They  are  (along  with  the  psychopathic  personalities)  the  most 
consciously  disidentified  with  their  mothers  and  their  fathers. 

6.  They  tend  as  a  group  to  be  extremely  disidentified  with  their 
spouses. 

7.  They  (along  with  the  psychopathic  personalities)  show  a  ten- 
dency to  misperceive  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  others.  They  are 
inclined  to  attribute  too  much  hostility  to  others. 

8.  Considering  all  the  eight  diagnostic  types  (at  Level  I),  the 
schizoid  group  comprises  the  largest  number  of  unmarried  individuals. 
This  suggests  that  more  schizoid  patients  than  patients  of  any  other 
diagnostic  type  have  failed  to  accomplish  a  durable,  conventional  mat- 
ing relationship. 

9.  The  schizoid  personality  tends  to  appear  in  certain  cultural  and 
institutional  samples  much  more  frequently  than  others.  The  per- 
centage of  rebellious  individuals  (Level  I-M)  in  various  samples  is 
presented  in  Table  16.  The  percentage  figure  expected  by  chance  for 
these  groups  is  12.5.  It  will  be  noted  that  eight  groups  contain  more 
than  or  close  to  the  expected  number  of  rebellious-distrustful  person- 
alities— the  four  psychiatric  samples,  the  graduate  student,  the  overtly 
neurotic  dermatitis,  the  prisoner,  and  the  psychotic  samples.  All  but 
two  of  these  define  "people  in  trouble,"  i.e.,  at  odds  with  or  malad- 
justed to  society.  The  fact  that  the  graduate  student  sample  contains 
a  higher  percentage  of  schizoid  personalities  than  the  more  conven- 
tional groups  suggests  that  rebelliousness  is  a  characteristic  of  this  sam- 
ple. This  is  an  interesting  confirmation  of  the  hypothesis  that  creativ- 
ity, delinquency,  and  alienation  involve  somewhat  similar  security 


28o  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


TABLE  16 

Percentage  of  Rebellious-Distrustful  Personalities  (Level  I-M) 

Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Rebellious-Distrustful 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission 

537 

12 

CoUege  Undergraduates 

415 

3 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic 

133 

22 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

121 

3 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

13 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

57 

7 

Unanxious  Dermatius  Patients 

71 

3 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 

109 

24 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

49 

12 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

0 

Ulcer  Patients 

43 

0 

Medical  Control  Patients 

J7 

3 

University  Counseling  Center 

93 

4 

University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 

39 

13 

Stockade  Prisoners   (Male) 

52 

10 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients 

28 

14 

Officers  in  Military  Service 

39 

3 

Total 

J903 

Operations — rebelliousness  toward  conventionality.  The  overtly  neu- 
rotic dermatitis  sample  differs  from  the  other  psychosomatic  groups 
in  the  amount  of  bitter,  masochistic  behavior  manifested.  Rebellious- 
distrustful  patients  comprise  the  largest  percentage  of  patients  who 
enter  and  remain  in  group  therapy  at  the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic. 
One  out  of  every  four  patients  (24  per  cent)  entering  group  therapy 
were  schizoid  personalities.  Two  factors  are  suggested  to  account  for 
this  finding.  The  clinic  intake  conference  has  found  that  group 
therapy  is  the  most  effective  therapeutic  agent  for  patients  who  are 
isolated,  distrustful,  and  distant  from  others.  Thus,  the  clinicians  re- 
fer more  schizoid  patients  to  groups.  Obsessives  and  phobic  patients, 
on  the  contrary,  are  more  likely  to  be  referred  to  individual  treatment. 

A  second  possible  explanation  for  this  finding  is  that  schizoid  per- 
sonalities tend  to  remain  in  therapy  because  the  diffused  and  diluted 
transference  phenomena  in  the  group  are  less  intense  than  the  trans- 
ference of  individual  therapy.  These  distrustful  people  can  apparently 
stand  the  interpersonal  pressure  in  the  groups  where  they  can  remain 
silent  or  sullen  for  considerable  periods  without  completely  disrupt- 
ing the  therapeutic  process. 

10.  The  percentage  of  subjects  who  diagnose  themselves  as  re- 
bellious-distrustful (Level  II-C)  is  presented  in  Table  17.  These  find- 
ings tend  to  be  in  line  with  the  Level  I-M  data  just  discussed.  Group 


%  of  Rebellious-Distrustful 

N 

Personalities 

207 

10 

46 

11 

101 

20 

38 

31 

13 

56 

70 

41 

42 

49 

0 

100 

0 

ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  REBELLION  281 


TABLE  17 

Percentage  of  Rebellious-Distrustful  Personalities  (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Savrple 
Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients   (Male) 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Panents 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patients 

Ulcer  Patients 

Hypertensive  Patients 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

Total  781 

psychotherapy  patients  again  comprise  the  largest  percentage  of 
schizoid  cases.  Overtly  neurotic  dermatitis  patients,  again,  differ  from 
the  other  psychosomatic  groups  in  the  emphasis  on  bitter  behavior. 

1 1 .  Schizoid  patients  consciously  perceive  their  parents  to  be  v/t2k 
and  distrustful  people.  The  mean  placement  of  fathers  of  schizoid 
patients  is  in  the  FG  section  of  the  diagnostic  grid.  Mothers  locate 
in  the  guilty,  self-punitive  {HI)  sector.  Schizoid  patients  report  their 
marital  partners  as  strong  and  exploitive. 

Rejerences 

1.  Lawrence,  D   H.  Aaron^s  Rod.   New  York-  Thomas  Seltzer,  1922. 

2.  Leary,  T.,  and  H.  Coffey.  Interpersonal  diagnosis:  Some  problems  of  methodol- 
ogy and  validauon.  /.  abnorm.  soc.  Psychol.,  1955,  SO,  No.  1,  110-25. 

3.  Lindner,  R.  Prescription  for  rebellion.  New  York:  Rinehart  Press,  1952. 

4.  Powdermaker,  Florence.  Concepts  found  useful  in  treatment  of  schizoid  and 
ambulatory  schizophrenic  patients.  Psychiat.,  1952,  25,  No.  1,  61-71. 

5.  Rosenthal,  D.,  J.  Frank  and  E.  Nash.  The  self-righteous  moralist  in  early  meet- 
ings of  therapeutic  groups.  Psychiat.,  1954,  11,  No.  3,  215-23. 

6.  Sullivan,  H.  S.  Therapeutic  investigations  on  schizophrenia.  In  P.  Mullahy 
(ed.),  A  study  of  interperso7ial  relations.  New  York:  Hermitage  House,  1949. 
Copyright,  New  York:  Thomas  Nelson  &  Sons. 


16 


Adjustment  Through  Self-Effacement: 
The  Masochistic  Personality 


We  are  considering  in  this  chapter  the  many  personality  types  which, 
despite  their  multilevel  differences,  have  one  important  thing  in  com- 
mon— they  all  present  in  their  overt  operations  a  fagade  of  self-efface- 
ment. This  is  the  ''55''  personality  type. 

The  message  which  they  communicate  to  others  in  their  face-to- 
face  relations  is  "I  am  a  weak,  inferior  person."  Through  their  auto- 
matic reflex  operations  they  train  others  to  look  down  upon  them 
with  varying  intensities  of  derogation  and  superiority. 

The  mild  form  of  this  security  operation  is  manifested  as  a  modest, 
unpretentious  reserve.  In  its  maladaptive  extremes  it  becomes  a  maso- 
chistic self-abasement.  In  either  case  the  person  employing  this  gen- 
eral mechanism  avoids  anxiety  by  means  of  retiring,  embarrassed 
diffidence.  He  is  automatically  mobilized  to  shun  the  appearance  of 
outward  strength  and  pride. 

The  Purpose  of  Self-Depreciation 

The  individuals  who  employ  this  security  operation  do  so  because 
they  feel  that  this  social  role  is  the  safest  and  least  dangerous  position 
to  be  assumed  in  this  particular  situation.  Now  persons  vary  in  the 
consistency  with  which  they  employ  any  interpersonal  behavior. 
Some  repetitiously  respond  with  the  same  reflexes  in  almost  all  situa- 
tions, whether  appropriate  or  not.  Others  may  automatically  assume 
modest,  retiring  reflexes  in  particular  situations  where  they  expect  it 
to  be  appropriate.  Many  subjects,  for  example,  act  embarrassed  and 
reserved  when  facing  strong  and  potentially  dangerous  others. 

In  this  chapter  we  are  considering  those  patients  who  present  a 
fagade  of  guilty  submissiveness  in  their  approach  to  the  clinic.  We 
cannot  assume,  of  course,  that  all  these  patients  act  in  this  wav  in  all 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  SELF-EFFACEMENT  283 

their  life  relationships.  We  simply  know  that  this  is  their  inter- 
personal impact  on  the  clinic.  It  is,  therefore,  the  aspect  of  their  per- 
sonality that  we  must  begin  to  respond  to  and  deal  with. 

Whenever  we  observe  or  measure  this  security  operation,  we  may 
assume  that  an  individual  has  learned  to  employ  self-depreciation  as  a 
protective  device  in  certain  situations,  or  in  all  situations.  Later  in- 
vestigation (e.g.,  measurements  at  other  levels)  will  indicate  the  range 
and  consistency  of  this  security  operation. 

The  role  of  masochism  in  contributing  to  the  security  of  the  indi- 
viduals has  been  pointed  out  by  several  psychoanalytic  authors. 
Menaker  has  contributed  an  excellent  summary  of  these  theories: 

The  observation  that  masochism  is  a  way  of  avoiding  anxiety,  a  point  on 
which  a  number  of  analysts  agree,  is  a  clue  to  the  fact  that  one  of  its  important 
aspects  is  its  function  of  defending  the  ego.  Important  psychoanalytic  contribu- 
tions to  the  understanding  of  masochism,  however,  have  thus  far  been  too  ex- 
clusively concerned  with  its  libidinal  meaning.  The  point  of  departure  has 
been  how  gratification  is  achieved  for  the  individual  through  masochistic  be- 
havior, rather  than  examining  the  way  in  which  it  serves  the  ego. 

We  find  that  viewing  the  problem  of  masochism  from  the  standpoint  of  the 
self-preservative  functions  of  the  ego  leads  to  new  insights.  As  might  be  ex- 
pected, the  ego  function  of  the  masochistic  attitude  is  most  clearly  discernible  in 
the  study  of  moral  masochism.  Berliner,  confining  his  observations  primarily 
to  moral  masochism,  has  made  an  important  contribution  to  the  concept  of 
masochism  as  a  defense  mechanism  of  the  ego.  He  takes  masochism  out  of  the 
sphere  of  the  instincts  and  views  it  as  a  function  of  the  ego.  It  is  'a  pathologic 
way  of  loving'  in  which  the  ego  through  processes  of  introjection,  identifica- 
tion and  superego  formation  turns  the  sadism  of  the  love  object  (not  its  own 
sadism)  on  itself.  The  motivation  for  so  doing  is  the  need  to  cling  to  a  vitally 
needed  love  object.  The  dependent  child  accepts  the  suffering  emanating  from 
the  rejecting  love  object  as  if  it  were  love,  failing  to  be  conscious  of,  or  denying 
the  difference  between,  love  and  hate.  Once  the  hating  love  object  has  become 
part  of  the  superego,  the  constant  wish  to  please  and  placate  the  superego  causes 
the  individual  to  lose  his  identity  and  to  'make  himself  as  unlovable  as  he  feels 
the  parent  wants  him  to  be.' 

Analytic  experience  confirms  Berliner's  view  of  masochism  as  a  function  of 
the  ego  in  the  service  of  maintaining  a  vitally  needed  love  relationship  to  a 
primary  object.   (4  pp.  207-8) 

The  general  purpose  of  the  masochistic  mechanism  seems  to  involve 
the  warding  off  of  anxiety  by  means  of  self-depreciation.  The  more 
specific  meanings  of  the  mechanism  vary  from  case  to  case  depending 
on  the  multilevel  pattern. 

The  fact  that  the  rather  shallow  methodology  of  the  interpersonal 
system  defines  several  thousand  types  which  express  masochism  at  one 
or  more  levels  of  personality  testifies  to  the  difficulty  of  making  broad 
generalizations  about  the  specific  meaning  of  masochism.  The  pattern 


284 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


of  conscious  and  "preconscious"  identifications  give  different  inteqjre- 
tations  of  self-punitive  behavior.  The  introjection  patterns  are  also 
crucial  in  some  cases.  We  have  developed  one  hypothesis  which  is  in 
line  with  the  psychoanalytic  theories  summarized  above.  It  seems 
logical  to  assume  that  wherever  masochism  is  expressed  at  any  level 
of  personality,  then  sadistic  feelings  are  also  present.  These  may  be 
attributed  to  the  conscious  or  "preconscious"  perceptions  of  others  or 
they  may  be  restricted  to  deeper  levels  of  "self-behavior."  Guilt  does 
not  exist  without  some  introjection  or  underlying  acceptance  of  puni- 
tive themes.  Self-criticism  seems  inevitably  to  involve  some  aspect  of 
hostile  criticism  expressed  against  or  projected  on  others. 

It  must  be  kept  clear  that  we  are  discussing  involuntary  reflexes  at 
this  point.  We  are  not  referring  to  the  conscious,  deliberate  assuming 
of  a  humble  role — nor  to  the  expression  of  modest  words  (i.e..  Level 
II  humility) ;  we  are  thinking  rather  of  automatic  tendencies  to  handle 
insecurity  by  means  of  weak,  depressive,  shy  operations. 

The  Effect  of  the  "'55''  Security  Operations 

Self-effacement  pulls  depreciation  and  patronizing  superiority  from 
others.  (In  the  code-language  of  the  interpersonal  system,  HI  pulls 
BC  and  DE  from  others.)  That  is  to  say,  if  a  person  acts  in  a  glum, 
guilty,  withdrawn,  and  weak  manner,  he  will  tend  to  train  others  to 
look  down  on  him  and  to  view  him  with  varying  amounts  of  contempt. 

One  interesting  expression  of  masochistic  behavior  which  invariably 
provokes  others  to  scorn  is  the  "buffoon"  personality.  One  psycho- 
analytic interpretation  of  the  interpersonal  meaning  of  the  clown's 
behavior  points  to  the  assumption  of  the  castrated  role.  According  to 
Grotjohn  (1)  the  clown  in  his  dress,  gestures,  and  thematic  expres- 
sions is  telling  the  audience:  "I  am  a  harmless,  weak,  defeated  person." 
The  social  buffoon  seems  to  exhibit  his  shameful,  inferior  position  and 
to  force  the  onlookers  to  laugh  at  him  and  to  patronize  him. 

The  reciprocal  interaction  does  not  occur  in  every  case.  The 
phenomenon  of  reciprocity  is,  as  we  have  seen  in  Chapter  7,  a  prob- 
ability statement.  Self-derogation  sometimes  pulls  initial  sympathy, 
but  if  the  guilty  reflex  does  not  shift  in  response  to  this  positive  re- 
action, the  "other  one"  will  inevitably  respond  with  irritation  and  dis- 
approval. Another  factor  preventing  the  reciprocal  process  from  in- 
variably working  resides  in  the  personality  of  the  "other  one."  If  a 
modest  person,  or  a  buffoon,  is  dealing  with  a  rigidly  docile  "other" 
— the  latter  may  not  respond  with  superiority  and  disdain.  In  general 
these  relationships  do  not  remain  durable  since  the  self-depreciator 
tends  to  gravitate  away  from  "equal"  relationships  and  to  prove  re- 
jection by  means  of  withdrawal.  The  docile  person  tends  also  to  avoid 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  SELF-EFFACEMENT  285 

equal  relationships  and  to  seek  strong,  guiding  partners.  If  two  indi- 
viduals with  submissive  fagades  maintain  a  durable  relationship,  it  will 
generally  be  found  that  a  reciprocity  of  underlying  themes  (often 
of  a  competitive  or  depreciatory  nature)  is  preserving  the  interaction. 

Self-abasing  individuals  provoke  punitive  and  arrogantly  superior 
reactions  from  others.  Most  persons  do  not  prefer  to  maintain  rela- 
tionships with  weak,  guilty  people.  They  tend  to  look  down  on  the 
masochists  when  they  encounter  them  and  do  not  enter  into  durable 
interactions. 

While  most  people  avoid  the  masochists,  there  are,  however,  some 
dramatic  exceptions  to  this  rule.  By  the  systematic  and  statistical  logic 
of  the  interpersonal  circle,  one  quarter  of  the  population  is  bound  to 
fall  into  the  upper  left-hand  quadrant.  This  is  the  area  which  includes 
the  operations  of  exploitation  (C),  narcissism  (B),  and  punitive  hos- 
tility (D).  These  individuals  provoke  fear,  envy,  and  guilt  in  others. 
The  modest  masochist,  we  have  seen,  trains  others  to  reject  and  despise 
him.  The  beautiful  interlocking  of  reciprocal  reflexes  which  occurs  in 
the  relationship  between  these  two  types  is,  of  course,  one  of  the 
most  familiar  problems  in  dynamic  psychology. 

Self-effacing,  guilty  individuals  feel  the  least  anxiety  when  they  are 
manifesting  their  masochistic  reflexes.  They  therefore  gravitate  to 
and  stay  with  those  individuals  who  will  provoke  the  least  anxiety — 
the  aggressive,  exploitive  characters  from  the  upper  left  part  of  the 
diagnostic  grid. 

This  reciprocal  phenomenon  is  seen  over  and  over  again  in  the  mal- 
adaptive masochistic  marriage — the  overtly  sorrowful,  martyred, 
abasive  wife  hopelessly  entangled  with  the  brutal  husband — or  the 
overtly  shy,  timid  man  wearing  himself  out  in  service  of  the  exploitive, 
narcissistic  wife. 

This  exchange  of  guilt  and  superiority  also  exists  with  remarkable 
frequency  in  the  relationships  of  normal,  adaptive  individuals.  One 
individual  takes  the  modest,  inferior,  self-eflFacing  part,  while  the 
other  exercises  the  superior  role — to  the  comfort  of  both.  Such 
reciprocal  relations  are  generally  complicated  by  underlying  motives. 
We  are  discussing  in  this  section  the  general  aspects  of  the  modest 
masochistic  security  technique  as  exhibited  in  overt  interpersonal  re- 
flexes. We  shall  therefore  postpone  the  detailed  discussion  of  the 
multilevel  patterns  which  usually  underly  the  self-abasive  fa9ade. 

Level  I  modesty  and  self-depreciation  can  be  adaptive  or  rigid,  mal- 
adaptive responses.  Their  purpose  is  to  ward  off  anxiety.  They  lead 
to  the  counterreactions  of  depreciation  and  superiority  on  the  part  of 
the  "other  one."  Extreme,  rigid  masochism  invariably  sets  up  new 
chains  of  conflict  and  increased  anxiety  which  can  be  responded  to  by 


286  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

increased  repetition  of  self-abasement,  by  related  symptomatology, 
and  by  other  signs  of  psychic  distress. 

We  shall  now  consider  some  of  the  clinical  manifestations,  both 
interpersonal  and  symptomatological,  of  the  modest-masochistic  per- 
sonality. 

Clinical  Definition  of  the  "55"  Personality 

The  symptomatic,  clinical  aspects  of  the  overtly  self-effacing  per- 
sonality are  easily  described.  The  outstanding  symptom  is  depression. 
These  people  are  overtly  anxious  and  unhappy.  They  exhibit  guilt 
and  self-depreciation.  Doubt,  rumination,  and  obsessive  uncertainty 
are  emphasized.  Associated  with  this  is  an  immobilized  passivity. 

They  are  not  active  or  self-confident.  They  are  not  assertive  or 
reasonable.  They  do  not  challenge  or  compete  with  the  clinician. 

Their  interpersonal  impact  on  others  involves  weakness.  They 
often  admit  their  need  for  psychotherapy.  They  tend  to  make  the 
clinician  feel  comfortable  in  his  role  because  tHey  readily  assume  the 
position  of  a  patient.  These  are  the  patients  who  keep  the  clinics  in 
business. 

These  patients  are  often  riddled  by  guilty,  obsessive  thinking. 
Hecht's  investigation  of  the  masochistic  personality  revealed  that  ob- 
sessive rumination  (as  measured  by  the  MMPI)  had  an  important  diag- 
nostic relationship  to  self-effacement  (2).  This  has  been  confirmed 
repeatedly  by  our  own  studies,  which  have  revealed  a  correlation  be- 
tween obsessive  thinking  and  self-depreciating  behavior. 

When  a  patient  comes  to  the  clinic  emphasizing  such  messages  as 
"People  are  mean  to  me,"  "I  have  done  wrong,"  "I  am  unworthy," 
and  "I  am  inferior,"  then  the  presenting  operations  of  masochism  can 
be  suspected.  The  effect  of  this  approach  is  to  make  the  other  one 
feel  strong,  slightly  superior,  perhaps,  and  initially  supportive.  The 
superior  reaction  of  the  clinician  is  often  bound  up  in  his  therapeutic 
role  so  that  he  may  not  be  aware  that  this  response  is  being  pulled 
from  him.  The  untrained  clinician  is  often  provoked  to  sympathetic 
gestures.  The  more  sophisticated  diagnostician  is  usually  struck  by 
the  force  of  the  self-punitive  superego. 

This  brings  us  to  another  aspect  of  this  personality  type — the  moral- 
istic quality  of  their  self-reproaches.  The  masochistic,  guilty  patient  is 
generally  obsessed  with  matters  of  "right  and  wrong"  and  measures 
himself  (to  his  own  disadvantage)  against  his  own  strict  ideals.  This 
point  is  clearly  demonstrated  by  the  finding  that  the  sector  of  the 
diagnostic  circle  which  defines  self-effacement  is  the  farthest  re- 
moved from  the  standard  ego-ideal  image  of  our  culture.  Their  be- 
havior is  rated  in  the  HI  sector  of  the  circle — whereas  the  ego  ideal  is 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  SELF-EFFACEMENT  287 

invariably  located  in  the  opposite  sectors.  These  patients  are  dissatis- 
fied with  themselves,  and  this  is,  of  course,  related  to  their  relatively- 
high  motivation  for  psychotherapy. 

In  their  social  demeanor  these  patients  typically  tend  to  be  silent, 
fearful,  and  unsociable.  In  the  moderately  self-effacing  person  this 
may  be  seen  as  a  modest  reserve.  In  severe  cases  it  becomes  a  marked 
withdrawal. 

Obsessive  Neurosis  and  Selj-Ejfacement 

The  self-effacing  personality  manifests  the  symptoms  of  depression, 
immobilization,  and  ruminative  self-doubt.  Patients  whose  overt  inter- 
personal behavior  is  masochistic  or  self-derogatory  are  often  given  the 
standard  psychiatric  diagnosis  of  obsessive  neurotic.  In  Chapter  12 
common  clinical  diagnostic  categories  were  compared  to  interpersonal 
types.  Evidence  was  cited  which  showed  that  the  HI  sector  of  the 
circle  at  Level  II  was  related  to  the  familiar  obsessive  category.  Re- 
search on  Level  I  behavior  has  confirmed  this  finding.  Patients  who 
were  diagnosed  by  fellow  group  patients  as  falling  in  the  masochistic 
sector  of  the  circle  invariably  manifested  the  symptoms  of  the  obses- 
sional disorder.  On  the  MMPI  these  patients  have  their  highest  scores 
on  the  depression  and  psychasthenia  scales — which  are  generally  seen 
as  diagnostic  of  obsessional  processes. 

Several  correlation  studies  between  MMPI  scales  and  Level  I  be- 
havior have  been  reported  (3).  When  the  depression  and  psychas- 
thenia scales  are  correlated  with  the  Level  I-S  vertical  index,  significant 
negative  correlations  with  dominance  are  consistently  obtained.  De- 
pression and  worry  are  related  to  passivity. 

There  is  considerable  research  evidence  pointing  to  a  relationship 
between  the  interpersonal  security  operation  of  masochism  and  ob- 
sessive symptoms  and  chnical  diagnosis  of  obsessive  neurosis.  In  addi- 
tion, there  are  some  theoretical  links  between  masochism  and  the  ob- 
sessive process.  When  this  relationship  was  first  suggested  by  our 
data,  there  was  considerable  question  on  the  part  of  the  research  staff, 
as  well  as  the  advising  clinicians,  as  to  the  accuracy  of  tying  masochism 
to  obsessiveness. 

Subsequent  diagnostic  work  has  tended  to  confirm  the  relationship 
and  has  shed  some  light  on  its  possible  theoretical  meaning.  The  link- 
ing factor  seems  to  concern  guilt  and  self-derogation.  It  is  generally 
accepted  that  the  obsessive  symptoms — rumination,  concern  with 
right  and  wrong,  self-doubt,  etc. — are  connected  with  guilt.  So  is 
masochism.  It  seems  to  make  clinical  and  theoretical  sense  that  self- 
effacement  is  the  interpersonal  expression,  and  obsessiveness  the  symp- 
tomatic expression  of  the  same  overt  security  operation. 


288  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

The  Obsessive-Cofnpulsive  Phenomenon 

The  relationship  between  the  interpersonal  security  operations  of 
self-effacement  and  the  standard  symptomatic  diagnosis  of  "obsessive" 
raises  an  interesting  terminological  issue.  Obsessions  have  to  do  with 
persistent  ideas,  intellectual  preoccupations,  doubts,  worries,  guilty 
thoughts.  These  generally  lead  to  inhibition  of  action — expressive, 
spontaneous  action  in  particular.  Obsessiveness  is  typically  accom- 
panied by  indecisiveness  and  depressive  immobilization.  The  inter- 
personal correlate  of  obsessiveness  is  modest  passivity  and  self-punitive 
timidity.  Worried  rumination  communicates  the  interpersonal  mes- 
sage, "I  am  unsure,  fearful,  self-doubting." 

Compulsions  have  generally  been  distinguished  from  obsessions. 
Compulsions  are  repetitive  activities,  e.g.,  promptness,  orderliness, 
precise  activity,  disciplined  behavior.  Compulsions  often  have  an 
interpersonal  impact  quite  different  from  obsessiveness.  Compulsive 
individuals  are  often  not  indecisively  immobilized;  they  expend  a  great 
deal  of  energy  in  exact,  demanding  action.  They  often  communicate 
not  an  interpersonal  message  of  doubt  or  fear,  but,  on  the  contrary, 
one  of  righteous  self-satisfaction,  pedantry,  and  superiority.  It  seems 
in  some  cases  that  when  compulsions  are  successfully  executed  they 
express  the  opposite  interpersonal  meaning  of  obsessive  behavior. 

Obsessive  and  compulsive  behavior  are  traditionally  linked  in 
psychiatric  terminology.  The  terms  are  often  used  synonymously. 
In  most  diagnostic  texts  the  two  are  considered  together  and  a  para- 
doxical mixture  of  symptomatic  cues  is  lumped  together.  Worried 
self-doubt  and  pedantic  superiority  are  often  cited  together  as  diag- 
nostic cues  for  the  same  personality  type.  From  the  standpoint  of 
descriptive  or  symptomatic  psychiatry  these  inconsistencies  do  not 
appear  too  striking;  but  when  they  are  viewed  from  the  position  of 
interpersonal  theory,  the  paradoxical  and  dichotomous  nature  of  the 
obsessive-compulsive  syndrome  comes  sharply  into  focus. 

The  interpersonal  meaning  of  successful  compulsivity  is,  "I  am 
right  and  superior."  The  interpersonal  meaning  of  pure  obsessiveness 
is,  "I  am  wrong  and  unsure." 

The  general  practice  of  combining  these  two  opposing  security 
operations  is  a  confusing  and  inefficient  terminological  practice.  The 
functional  meaning  of  rigid  compulsivity  is  quite  different  from  that  of 
pure  obsessiveness,  and  different  from  both  of  these  are  the  many 
cases  which  show  alternations  of  both  behaviors.  Obsessive-compul- 
sive is  hyphenated  because  the  two  elements  appear  to  be  opposing, 
dichotomous  factors;  they  are  diametrically  different  ways  of  han- 
dling guilt  and  weakness.  Sado-masochism  is  another  familiar  hyphe- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  SELF-EFFACEMENT  289 

nated  term  in  psychiatry.  These  two  elements  are  also  Hnked  because 
they  are  diametrically  opposite  ways  of  dealing  with  hostility.  Clini- 
cians seem  to  recognize,  however,  that  although  sadism  and  masochism 
are  reciprocally  related,  the  two  words  are  not  synonymous.  It  is  of 
crucial  importance  to  know  which  side  of  a  sado-masochistic  conflict 
is  overt  and  which  is  underlying.  The  functional  problems  involved 
in  getting  an  overtly  sadistic  personality  into  therapy  are  quite  dis- 
tinct from  those  involving  the  overt  masochist.  The  latter  is  often 
initially  better  motivated. 

It  is  useful  to  make  the  same  distinction  in  the  case  of  the  obsessive- 
compulsive  phenomenon — that  is,  to  determine  specifically  whether 
a  patient  is  presenting  overtly  as  an  obsessive,  guilt-ridden,  depressed 
person,  or  whether  compulsive  defenses  are  successfully  operating. 
In  the  latter  case  the  patient  is  outwardly  active,  more  self-confident, 
and  manifests  a  righteous,  active  fa9ade. 

Many  cases  seen  in  the  psychiatric  clinic  show  mixtures  of  obses- 
sive-compulsive symptoms.  In  the  interpersonal  language  they  may 
be  guilty  and  self-effacing  at  Level  I-M,  but  this  may  be  seen  as  a 
temporary  breakdown  of  a  compulsive  personality.  Often  Level  I-M 
may  be  depressed  and  masochistic  while  the  Level  II  self-description 
emphasizes  managerial,  responsible  themes.  This  indicates  that  the 
compulsive  defenses  are  weakening;  guilt  and  weakness  in  the  form 
of  symptoms  are  breaking  through. 

Arthur  Kobler  of  the  Pinel  Foundation  Hospital  has  added  an  im- 
portant qualification  to  the  point  being  made  in  this  section.  He  be- 
lieves that  the  distinction  between  the  interpersonal  implication  of 
obsessive  versus  compulsive  behavior  may  hold  for  the  popular,  ad- 
justing aspect  of  compulsivity.  He  states,  however,  that  severe  com- 
pulsive rituals — "driven  actions  with  magical  quality" — are  closer  to 
obsessiveness.  The  interpersonal  theory  would  be  in  strong  agree- 
ment with  this  statement  because  it  interprets  these  bizarre  rituals  as 
diagnostic  of  the  schizoid  message,  "I  am  different,  queer,  alienated." 
Since  schizoid  behavior  in  the  interpersonal  system  falls  next  to  ob- 
sessiveness on  the  diagnostic  continuum,  Kobler's  valuable  clarification 
seems  to  fit  the  "circle"  theory. 

To  summarize:  The  distinction  between  compulsive  and  obsessive 
behavior  is  functionally  valuable.  Pure  compulsivity  (where  there  is 
no  breakthrough  of  the  warded-off,  underlying  guilt)  indicates  inter- 
personal power,  pedantry,  and  self-righteousness.  Pure  obsessiveness 
is  associated  with  overt  interpersonal  passivity  and  humiUty.  It  is 
possible  to  use  the  hyphenated  term  obsessive-compulsive  to  refer  to 
multilevel  patterns  of  conflict,  but  the  meaning  (symptomatic  and 
interpersonal)  of  the  separate  terms  should  be  kept  distinct. 


290 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the 
Self-Effacing-Masochistic  Personality 

The  characteristics  of  the  masochistic  personahty  which  have  just 
been  discussed  are  based  on  research  findings  of  the  Kaiser  Founda- 
tion project.  These  have  been  described  in  other  publications.  Some 
of  these  findings  will  now  be  summarized. 

1.  Patients  who  exhibit  masochistic  operations  at  Levels  I  and  II 
do  not  tend  to  have  psychosomatic  disorders,  except  for  the  overtly 
neurotic  dermatological  symptom  groups  (acne,  seborrheic  dermatitis, 
and  psoriasis) . 

2.  Psychosomatic  patients  do  not  present  self-punitive  behavior  in 
their  overt  operations,  except  for  the  above-listed  skin  disorders. 

3.  Patients  who  express  masochism  at  Level  I  tend  to  have  MMPI 
profiles  emphasizing  obsessive  (Pt),  depressive  (D),  and  passive  (Mf) 
trends. 

4.  Self-punitive  behavior  at  Level  II  is  also  related  to  the  same 
MMPI  scales. 

5.  These  patients  tend  to  stay  in  psychotherapy  longer  than  hys- 
teric, managerial,  narcissistic,  or  psychosomatic  patients.  They  tend 
to  stay  in  therapy  about  the  same  length  of  time  as  schizoid,  phobic, 
and  psychopathic  personalities.  They  belong  to  the  well-motivated 
group  of  patients. 


TABLE  18 

Percentage  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic  Personalities  (Level  I-M) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic 


Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Persona 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission 

537 

13 

College  Undergraduates 

415 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic 

133 

14 

Aliddle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

121 

Neurodermatitis  Cases 

112 

13 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

16 

Self-inflicted   Dermatitis   Patients 

57 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

71 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

49 

22 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

Ulcer  Patients 

43 

Medical  Control  Patients 

37 

University  Counseling  Center 

93 

University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 

39 

0 

Stockade  Prisoners  (Male) 

52 

2 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients 

28 

11 

Officers  in  Military  Service 

39 

0 

Total 

1903 

ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  SELF-EFFACEMENT  291 

6.  They  tend  to  be  consciously  disidentified  with  their  mothers. 

7.  They  tend  to  be  consciously  disidentified  with  their  fathers. 

8.  They  tend  to  be  consciously  disidentified  with  their  spouses. 

9.  The  masochistic  personality  is  found  most  frequently  in  certain 
institutional  and  cultural  settings  (see  Table  18).  Masochists  appear 
more  often  in  psychiatric  samples  and  rarely  occur  in  psychosomatic 
or  normal  samples.  One  exception  to  this  statement — certain  neuro- 
dermatitis groups  are  more  often  masochistic  (at  Level  I)  than  any 
other  psychosomatic  sample. 

10.  The  percentage  of  self-effacing  personalities  (defined  by  Level 
II-C)  found  in  various  samples  is  presented  in  Table  19. 

TABLE  19 

Percentage  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic  Personalities   (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Self-Effacing-Masochistic 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patierits 
Ulcer  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 
Total 

Neurotics  and  neurodermatitis  patients  express  the  most  masochism. 
Psychosomatic  and  normal  groups  the  least.  It  is  of  interest  that  the 
psychotic  group  manifests  considerably  less  self-effacement  than  the 
neurotic  samples.  The  impHcations  of  these  findings  are  discussed  in 
Chapters  23  and  24. 

References 

1.  Grotjohn,  M.  Jake  Gimbel  lectures,  University  of  CaUfomia,  1955. 

2.  Hecht,  Shirley.    An  investigation  into  the  psychology  of  masochism.    Unpub- 
lished doctor's  dissertation,  University  of  California,  1950. 

3.  Leary,  T.,  and  H.  Coffey.   The  prediction  of  interpersonal  behavior  in  group 
psychotherapy.  Psychodrama  gr.  psychother.  Monogr.,  1955,  No.  28. 

4.  Menaker,  Esther.  Masochism— a  defense  reaction  of  the  ego.  Psychoanal.  Quart., 
1953,  22,  No.  2,  205-20. 


207 

9 

46 

6 

101 

14 

38 

21 

31 

16 

56 

7 

70 

4 

41 

5 

42 

2 

49 

0 

100 

3 

781 

17 

Adjustment  Through  Docility: 
The  Dependent  Personality 


This  chapter  is  concerned  with  those  individuals  who  present  in  their 
approach  to  the  clinic  a  fa9ade  of  dependent,  docile  conformity.  This 
is  the  ''66''  personality  type.  The  interpersonal  message  it  conveys  to 
others  is,  "I  am  a  meek,  admiring  person  in  need  of  your  help  and 
advice." 

The  moderate  form  of  this  security  operation  is  expressed  as  a  re- 
spectful or  poignant  or  trustful  conformity.  In  its  maladaptive  inten- 
sity it  is  manifested  as  a  helpless  dependency.  These  subjects  in  their 
interpersonal  reflexes  avoid  the  expression  of  hostility,  independence, 
and  power. 

The  Purpose  of  Docile  Conformity 

Human  beings  utilize  these  security  operations  because  they  have 
found  that  they  are  least  anxious  when  they  are  outwardly  relying  on 
or  looking  up  to  others.  Some  individuals  employ  these  reflexes  in 
their  relationships  with  everyone  they  contact.  Others  assume  this 
role  when  they  assume  it  to  be  called  for  by  the  situation.  They  act 
helpless  and  fearful  when  dealing  with  strong  individuals,  authority 
figures,  and  the  like. 

Many  patients  automatically  assume  this  role  in  approaching  medi- 
cal or  therapeutic  agents.  The  doctor-patient  relationship  is  loaded 
with  dependency  implications.  Most  patients  manifest  a  certain 
amount  of  helpless  trust  in  coming  for  diagnosis.  The  normative  sta- 
tistics employed  in  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  grids  are  based  on 
large  samples  of  clinic  patients.  In  this  chapter  therefore  we  shall  be 
describing  those  individuals  who  express  more  dependency  than  the 
average  clinic  visitor.  We  have  isolated  these  persons  who  seem  to  go 
out  of  their  way  to  pull  sympathy,  help,  and  direction  from  others; 

292 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  DOCILITY  293 

who  use  their  symptoms  to  communicate  a  helpless,  painful,  uncertain, 
frightened,  hopeful,  dependent  passivity. 

Now  many  of  these  patients  exert  this  interpersonal  pressure  in  the 
clinic  but  may  act  quite  differently  in  other  situations.  They  may  be 
fairly  independent  in  certain  social  interactions.  At  other  levels  of 
behavior  they  may  be  less  phobic  and  docile.  The  fact  that  they  pre- 
sent dependence  as  their  calling  card  to  the  clinic  is  an  indication  that 
this  is  the  functionally  critical  point  at  which  to  begin  the  diagnostic 
evaluation.  This  defines  their  initial  motivation,  their  first  line  of  de- 
fense. 

Whenever  we  observe  this  security  operation,  we  tend  to  assume 
that  the  patient  has  come  to  employ  docile  dependence  as  a  means  of 
handling  anxiety  in  this  type  of  situation.  Further  investigations  may 
reveal  the  flexibility  or  rigidity  of  this  behavior  and  may  indicate 
that  opposing  motives  exist  at  other  levels  or  in  other  situations. 

The  Effect  of  Docile  Conformity  Upon  Others 

Docility  pulls  strong,  helpful  leadership  from  others.  Dependence 
provokes  nurturance.  In  the  language  of  the  circle,  ''JK  pulls  AP  and 
NO  from  others." 

If  a  person  acts  in  a  poignant,  helpless,  respectful  manner,  he  trains 
others  to  offer  help,  advice,  and  direction.  He  who  asks  tends  to  get 
taught.  These  subjects  tell  others  by  means  of  their  reflexes  that  they 
are  weak-and-friendly.  They  thereby  provoke  others  to  be  strong- 
and-friendly. 

These  reciprocal  tendencies  do  not  occur  inevitably  but  within 
probability  limits.  Some  punitive  individuals  react  with  stern  disap- 
proval to  dependence  in  another.  Severe  masochists  are  unable  to  ex- 
press nurturance  even  though  the  other  is  exerting  intense  dependent 
pressure.  In  general,  however,  docile  individuals  tend  to  be  most  com- 
fortable when  they  are  involved  with  strong,  responsible  individuals. 
Nurturant  people  naturally  seek  admiring,  trustful  individuals  who 
will  respond  to  and  need  their  help.  The  docile  phobic  person  tends  to 
irritate  the  rebel  and  to  threaten  the  counterphobic;  he  does  not  gen- 
erally integrate  durable  relations  with  these  individuals.  Close  sym- 
biotic ties  link  the  meek,  admiring  (JK)  individual  to  respected,  help- 
ful (APNO)  partners. 

These  reciprocal  situations  hold  for  brief  encounters  as  well  as 
durable  interpersonal  pairings.  Poignant,  tearful  helplessness  in  the 
first  few  seconds  of  an  interaction  provokes  tenderness  and  guidance 
from  another.  Patients  who  present  these  reflexes  in  an  initial  psychia- 
tric interview  generate  forces  which  may  tend  to  pull  assurance  from 
the  clinician.    Whenever  the  clinical  interviewer  finds  himself  un- 


294  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

usually  inspired  to  help,  to  promise,  to  reassure,  to  explain,  to  do  some- 
thing to  relieve  anxiety  and  tears,  he  will  generally  find  that  he  is  deal- 
ing with  security  operations  of  dependent  docility. 

The  effect  of  JK  behavior  is,  therefore,  to  train  the  "other  one"  to 
assume  a  strong,  friendly  role.  Circular  chains  of  interaction,  of  course, 
develop.  The  respected,  responsible,  nurturant  person  in  turn  presses 
the  dependent  person  to  increased  dependence.  Where  these  sym- 
biotic tendencies  are  uncomplicated  by  underlying  conflicts  on  the 
part  of  either  partner,  a  most  comfortable  durable  relationship  de- 
velops. The  passive  son  attached  to  a  strong  nurturant  mother  pro- 
vides a  typical  example  of  this  process.  The  docile,  adoring  wife 
dutifully  tied  to  a  responsible,  managerial  husband  is  another. 

Where  the  docility  is  intense  and  all  other  reflexes  are  crippled  or 
where  underlying  motives  conflict  with  the  overt  dependence,  then 
anxiety  fails  to  be  warded  ofl^.  This  anxiety  can  be  dealt  with  by  in- 
creased helplessness,  eventually  leading  to  a  fairly  typical  set  of  psy- 
chological symptoms.  The  neurotic  expressions  of  severe  dependent 
conformity  will  now  be  considered. 

Clinical  Definition  of  the  ''66"  Personality 

The  defensive  operations  of  docile  conformity,  when  employed  in 
the  intense  maladaptive  degree,  result  in  a  set  of  specific  symptoms 
which  are  related  to  and  a  logical  outcome  of  the  tactics. 

The  first  clinical  indication  is  helplessness  and  overt  anxiety.  Clini- 
cally this  is  generally  expressed  as  a  marked  depression.  Fears,  wor- 
ries, elaborate  concern  over  physical  or  emotional  discomfort  are 
common. 

A  most  definitive  sign  of  this  personality  type  is  the  presence  of 
phobias.  The  patient  is  fearful  of  events  or  experiences  without  any 
direct  rational  cause.  Descriptive  psychiatry  of  the  last  century  has 
listed  dozens  of  impressive-sounding  hyphenated  terms  denoting  the 
different  phobic  reactions.  While  it  seems  fruitless  to  recapitulate  this 
list  of  descriptive  labels,  it  seems  worth  while  to  point  out  that  they 
generally  refer  to  an  irrational  and  inexplicable  intense  fear  of  some 
stimulus — fear  of  heights,  fear  of  crowds,  fear  of  being  alone,  etc. 

The  theory  of  the  interpersonal  circle  offers  one  possible  rational 
correlation  of  these  fears  with  docile-dependent  operations.  Fears,  of 
course,  tend  to  give  the  impression  of  weakness  and  helplessness.  This 
pulls  for  help  and  support. 

But  the  fears  of  the  phobic  are,  in  essence,  displaced  fears.  It  is  well 
known  that  the  relatively  innocuous  stimuli  avoided  by  these  patients 
generally  stand  for  more  directly  intimate  interpersonal  figures  who 
are  covertly  feared.  We  recall  that  Little  Hans's  panic  about  horses 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  DOCILITY  295 

was  related  to  certain  unconscious  perceptions  of  his  father  and 
mother.  (2)  A  repressive  tendency  is  at  work  here.  The  patient  can- 
not directly  attribute  hostile,  dangerous  motives  to  real,  known  figures 
but  unconsciously  displaces  these  motives  to  figures  or  stimuli  which 
are  vague,  and  psychologically  distant.  One  effect  of  this  is  to  allow 
the  patient  to  preserve  a  consciously  conforming,  docile  relationship 
with  close  figures  against  whom  he  may  feel  negative,  rebellious  emo- 
tions. 

At  this  point  the  theory  of  the  interpersonal  circle  can  be  intro- 
duced. The  points  /  and  K  which  define  the  phobic  personality  are 
midway  between  weakness  (HI)  and  conventional  agreeability  (LM). 
On  the  circular  diagnostic  continuum,  the  phobic  is  related  on  the  one 
hand  to  the  obsessive  and  on  the  other  to  the  hysteric.  Clinically  this 
suggests  that  phobics  combine  fears  and  obsessions  on  the  one  hand 
with  a  conventional,  repressive,  bland  tendency  to  see  family  mem- 
bers and  intimates  as  sweet  and  loving.  The  MMPI  pattern  for  the 
phobic  personality  involves  peak  scores  on  depression,  psychostenia, 
and  hysteria.  This  tends  to  confirm  the  clinical  impression  of  a  per- 
son who  is  unhappy,  anxious  (D),  worried,  and  fearful  (Pt),  and  at 
the  same  time  blandly  repressive  (Hy).  He  is  afraid,  but  he  does  not 
know  what  he  fears.  He  is  helpless  and  weak  within  the  context  of 
docile,  naive  conformity. 

So  far  we  have  emphasized  the  phobic  symptoms  of  the  docile-de- 
pendent personality.  The  point  has  been  made  that  inexplicable  fears 
seem  to  fit  nicely  the  mixture  of  weakness  plus  bland  conventionality 
which  characterizes  this  personality  type.  There  are  other  symptoms 
which  allow  the  patient  to  be  helpless,  depressed,  and  anxious,  and  to 
maintain  a  conforming  conscious  picture  of  self  and  others.  Diffuse 
physical  symptoms,  for  example,  have  the  same  psychological  implica- 
tions. They  tend  to  be  typical  of,  and  partially  diagnostic  of,  the 
docile  phobic  personahty. 

We  are  considering  here  symptoms  which  seem  to  be  physical  ex- 
pressions of  anxiety  and  tension;  insomnia,  transient  digestive  com- 
plaints (e.g.,  "butterflies  in  stomach"  and  nausea  or  bowel  reactions  in 
response  to  stress)  and  transient  circulatory  symptoms  (blushing,  faint- 
ing behavior,  cardiac  responses  to  stress,  etc.).  Many  diffuse  hypo- 
chondriacal concerns  have  docile-dependent  overtones. 

The  interpersonal  function  of  these  symptoms  is  to  present  a  pic- 
ture of  a  worried,  distressed  person  in  need  of  help  because  of  symp- 
toms which  have  an  indirect  emotional  significance.  The  patient  suf- 
fering from  diffuse  physical  symptoms  often  does  not  complain  spe- 
cifically or  directly  about  his  interpersonal  problems  or  those  of  his 
intimates  but  displaces  much  of  his  concern  onto  areas  which  are 


2^6  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

psychologically  more  distant  and  much  more  indirect.   Again,  he  is 
anxious  but  he  does  not  know  why. 

The  third  and  most  pathonomonic  set  of  symptoms  characteristic  of 
the  docile-dependent  personality  includes  the  manifestations  of  overt, 
free-floating  anxiety.  When  a  patient  comes  to  the  clinic  openly  ex- 
pressing signs  of  weakness,  discomfort,  concern  over  self  (e.g.,  tears, 
fidgeting,  fearful  behavior),  then  the  security  operations  of  phobic 
conformity  may  be  suspected.  The  interpersonal  message  expressed 
by  these  tactics  seems  to  be:  "I  am  a  distressed,  weak,  unhappy  person 
in  need  of  your  help  and  direction."  This  and  the  preceding  generali- 
zations refer  to  the  generalized  or  pure  or  consistent  case.  Alany  pa- 
tients manifest  alternations  of  behavior  in  a  diagnostic  interview.  They 
may  initially  exhibit  interpersonal  reflexes  of  self-confident  superior 
strength  and  then  lapse  suddenly  into  fearful,  tearful  behavior.  In 
this  case  the  hypothesis  of  intense  phobic-counterphobic  conflict 
would  perhaps  be  considered. 

Relationship  of  Docile  Conformity  to  Standard 
Psychiatric  Diagnosis 

The  preceding  section  has  suggested  that  the  interpersonal  traits  of 
overt  docile  dependence  are  related  to  certain  clinical  symptoms. 
These  were  anxiety,  phobias,  and  diffuse  physical  symptoms. 

Patients  who  employ  these  operations  and  manifest  these  symptoms 
can  be  given  five  difl^erent  standard  psychiatric  diagnoses.  The  diag- 
nostic label  used  is  generally  determined  by  the  kind  of  symptoms 
which  characterize  the  patient. 

1.  The  term  anxiety  neurosis  generally  defines  a  docile-dependent 
personality.   Malamud  describes  this  diagnostic  type  as  follows: 

Clinically,  this  disturbance  expresses  itself  in  attacks  of  vague,  unexplained 
but  intense  fear  which,  at  least  in  the  beginning  of  the  disease,  does  not  seem 
to  be  attached  to  any  particular  object.  It  can  best  be  described  as  being  near 
to  a  normal  fear  of  a  vital  danger,  but  is  different  from  it  in  that  no  such  dan- 
ger is  present  and,  in  most  cases,  not  even  imagined  to  be  present.  The  con- 
comitant symptoms  are  usually  of  the  same  kind  as  found  in  real  fear— a  kind  of 
paralyzed  state  of  the  musculature,  cold  shivers,  a  sense  of  pressure  in  the  head 
and  precordial  regions,  profuse  cold  sweating,  palpitation  of  the  heart,  and  at 
times  relaxation  of  the  sphincters.  As  time  goes  on  the  attacks  may  be  con- 
sciously associated  with  some  of  the  concomitant  symptoms.  The  person  may 
develop  the  fear  that  his  heart  may  stop,  that  something  will  burst  in  his  head, 
or  that  some  serious  disease  is  developing  in  his  gastro-intestinal  system.  The 
concomitant  symptoms  may  also  assume  the  controlling  feature  of  the  picture, 
and  thus  instead  of  pure  anxiety  attacks  we  may  have  tachycardia,  alternating 
constipation  or  diarrhea,  dizziness,  or  even  vertigo,  and  others.   (3,  p.  853) 

The  patient  who  complains  of  these  symptoms  usually  approaches 
the  clinician  in  a  dependent  manner,  seeking  relief  and  help. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  DOCILITY  297 

2.  The  term  phobic  is  also  used  to  describe  docile  patients.  Again 
the  interpersonal  implication  of  the  fearful  state  is  that  the  patient  is 
a  weak,  helpless  person.  There  is  considerable  overlap  in  the  descrip- 
tions of  anxiety  neurotics  and  phobics.  Notice  in  Malamud's  definition 
the  emphasis  placed  on  fears.  The  difference  between  anxiety  neurosis 
and  phobia  seems  to  involve  superficial  descriptive  aspects  of  the  con- 
tent of  what  is  feared.  Both  types  seem  to  describe  the  same  generic 
personality  syndrome. 

3.  In  the  Freudian  literature  a  similar  overlapping  of  terms  occurs. 
In  defining  anxiety  hysteria  Fenichel  states  that  "the  anxiety  is  spe- 
cifically connected  with  a  special  situation,  which  represents  the  neu- 
rotic conflict.  (1,  p.  194)  In  discussing  "the  choice  of  the  specific 
content"  of  the  fears  in  anxiety  hysteria,  Fenichel  (1,  p.  195)  moves 
immediately  to  a  consideration  of  phobias,  and  it  is  clear  that  he  con- 
siders phobias  the  characteristic  symptom  of  the  anxiety  hysteric. 

It  appears  that  the  terms  anxiety  and  phobic  as  used  in  diagnostic 
labels  are  descriptive  and  symptomatic.  The  value  of  these  terms  for 
nosology  is  limited.  One  is  led  to  question  (1)  the  usefulness  of 
descriptive  diagnostic  labels  and  (2)  the  proliferation  of  these  over- 
lapping terms. 

4.  There  is  a  fourth  standard  diagnostic  category  which  is  related 
to  the  docile-dependent  personality.  This  is  the  term  neurasthenic. 
Malamud  gives  a  description  of  this  condition: 

In  its  pure  form  it  is  characterized  by  feelings  of  physical  and  mental  in- 
adequacy, complaints  of  fatigability  without  adequate  exertion,  paresthesias  in 
the  back  of  the  neck,  and  a  sense  of  general  weakness.  In  the  more  chronic  and 
severe  forms  of  this  disturbance  the  patients  usually  describe  themselves  as 
mental  and  physical  "wrecks."  They  cannot  concentrate  on  any  activity,  they 
wake  up  in  the  morning  feeling  exhausted,  "fagged  out,"  unable  to  get  started 
on  any  work.  Irritability,  feeling  of  lack  of  sexual  vigor  at  times  amounting  to 
impotence,  and  a  vague  sense  of  anxiety  may  complicate  the  picture.  In  contra- 
distinction to  the  anxiety  neuroses,  these  states  are  usually  monotonously 
chronic  without  any  great  degree  of  variation  and  as  is  too  frequently  the  case 
the  patient  seeks  for  help  only  after  long  duration  of  the  symptoms.  (3,  p.  854) 

The  similarity  of  neurasthenia  to  anxiety  neurosis  is  apparent  in 
this  description  and  is,  in  fact,  recognized  by  Malamud.  It  seems  that 
the  differentiating  factor  is  chronicity — a  dubious  reason  for  retaining 
a  nosological  category. 

5.  A  fifth  diagnostic  term  which  usually  defines  the  overtly  de- 
pendent personality  is  hypochondriasis.  Diffuse  physical  symptoms 
and  worry  about  bodily  functions  can  serve  as  a  defense  in  many  types 
of  maladjustment.  Often  these  concerns  operate  in  very  sick  patients 
to  ward  off  psychotic  processes.  It  is  safe  to  say  that  in  any  hypo- 
chondriacal condition,  whatever  the  underlying  problem,  displace- 


298  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

ment  and  repressive  processes  are  at  work.  At  the  level  of  presenta- 
tion to  the  clinic  the  interpersonal  implications  of  hypochondriasis  are 
(1)  dependence  and  need  for  help  and  (2)  some  tendency  to  displace 
negative  emotions  onto  physical  reactions.  These  two  factors  are 
characteristic  of  the  personality  type  we  are  discussing  in  this  chapter. 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the ''66''  Personality 

This  section  presents  a  summary  of  some  of  the  empirical  studies 
accomplished  on  the  phobic  personality  at  the  Kaiser  Foundation  re- 
search. 

1 .  Patients  who  present  docile  conformity  in  their  overt  operations 
do  not  tend  to  have  the  psychosomatic  symptoms  of  ulcer,  hyperten- 
sion, or  neurodermatitis.  (Although  they  do  not  manifest  organ 
neuroses,  they  do  tend  to  complain  of  diffuse  physical  symptoms  of 
anxiety.) 

2.  Docile  dependency  at  Levels  I  and  II  is  related  to  depression 
(D),  ruminative  worries  (Pt),  and  naive  blandness  (Hy)  on  the 
MMPI. 

3.  These  patients  tend  to  be  initially  well  motivated  for  treatment, 
remaining  in  treatment  for  an  average  of  eleven  interviews.  Pure 
phobics  (i.e.,  without  underlying  ambivalence)  remain  in  treatment 
an  average  of  twenty  times,  that  is,  longer  than  any  other  diagnostic 
group.  They  are  solidly  docile  and  dependent.  Conflicted  phobics 
(i.e.,  with  underlying  hostility,  strength,  or  conventionality)  on  the 
contrary  do  not  remain  in  treatment,  being  seen  on  the  average  of  2.6 
sessions.  This  dramatic  reversal  of  the  pure  and  the  more  ambivalent 
cases  points  up  the  necessity  of  fitting  the  variability  dimension  into 
the  diagnostic  picture. 

4.  Docile  subjects  are  on  the  average  ambivalent  in  their  conscious 
identification  with  parents.  They  are  not  so  disidentified  as  the 
psychopaths,  schizoids,  and  obsessives.  They  are  less  close  to  their 
parents  than  the  conventional  and  responsible  personality  types. 

5.  They  are  similarly  about  in  the  middle  on  the  variable  of  marital 
identification,  being  closer  to  their  marital  partners  than  the  uncon- 
ventional diagnostic  groups  and  less  close  than  the  conventional. 

6.  Docile  patients  consciously  describe  their  parents  as  being  con- 
ventional, agreeable,  and  somewhat  nurturant  people.  This  reflects  a 
conforming  attitude  to  parents  and  places  them  close  to  the  hysterical 
and  psychosomatic  patients.  Phobics  picture  themselves  as  weaker 
than  the  latter  two  personality  types,  but  share  their  conventional  per- 
ception of  parents. 

7.  Phobics  emphasize  nurturance  in  their  conscious  description  of 
marital  partners  more  than  any  other  diagnostic  group.    They  are 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  DOQUTY  299 

themselves  dependent  and  marry  people  whom  they  see  as  strong  and 
giving. 

8.  They  are  therefore  not  identified  with  their  spouses  but  report 
reciprocal  "needs  help-gives  help"  marital  relationships. 

9.  On  the  Naboisek  study  of  interpersonal  misperception,  phobics 
(when  combined  with  obsessives)  seem  to  be  the  most  accurate  of  any 
diagnostic  type.  They  correctly  perceive  the  strong  to  be  strong  and 
the  weak  to  be  weak.  They  manifest  misperception  only  in  the  case 
of  the  hostile  persons,  to  whom  they  erroneously  attribute  more  weak- 
ness than  hostility.  Docile  dependent  patients  seem  to  be  thrown  off 
by  aggressiveness  in  others,  preferring  to  see  this  as  weakness.  This 
may  reflect  an  avoidance  of  the  same  interpersonal  themes  they  avoid 
in  their  overt  behavior. 

10.  The  phobic  personality  is  found  most  frequently  in  certain  in- 
stitutional and  cultural  settings.  Docile  people  (Level  I-M)  do  come 
to  the  psychiatric  clinic  for  help.  They  are  not  found  as  frequently 
in  normal  nonclinical  settings  (see  Table  20). 

TABLE  20 

Percentage  of  Docile-Dependent  Personalities   (Level  I-M) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Docile-Dependent 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions  537  12 

College  Undergraduates  415  2 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic  133  10 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female)  121  5 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients  31  10 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients  57  5 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients  71  3 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients  109  11 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients  49  16 

f lypertensive  Patients  49  5 

Ulcer  Patients  43  5 

Medical  Control  Patients  57  5 

University  Counseling  Center  (Male)  93  2 

University  Graduate  Students  (Male)  39  5 

Stockade  Prisoners  (Male)  52  6 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  28  21 

Officers  in  Military  Service  39  0 

Total  1903 

11.  Docile-dependent  patients  tend  to  be  assigned  to  individual 
psychotherapy.  They  are  second  only  to  the  obsessives  in  the  per 
cent  referred  to  and  remaining  in  individual  treatment.  They  are  not 
referred  as  frequently  to  group  therapy.  They  do  not  remain  in  group 


300  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

psychotherapy  as  frequently.  Four  other  diagnostic  types  supply 
more  patients  who  remain  in  groups.  The  phobics  top  only  the  hys- 
terics, psychopaths,  and  narcissists  in  percentage  of  group  therapy 
numbers.  This  is  probably  due  to  the  following  facts:  The  phobic 
tends  to  be  quite  ambivalent  about  treatment  in  general.  He  wants 
help  but  not  necessarily  psychological  exploration.  When  he  is  seen 
individually,  this  ambivalence  about  motivation  and  commitment  to 
therapy  can  be  made  the  focus  of  attention  and  dealt  with  directly. 
The  docile  patient  is  more  likely  to  feel  comfortable  in  a  two-way 
doctor-patient  relationship.  The  dependent  operations  work  more 
smoothly.  In  a  group  the  ambivalent  motivation  is  very  easy  to  over- 
look. The  patient  does  not  have  a  single  comfortable  situation  of  a 
nurturant  therapist  but  is  thrown  into  interaction  with  several  other 
patients  and  personality  types.  Underlying  ambivalences  can  be 
intensified  and  the  phobic  often  drops  out  of  the  group. 

12.  The  frequency  of  docile-dependent  subjects  at  Level  II-C  is 
presented  in  Table  21.  It  will  be  noted  that  this  personality  type  is 
again  most  numerous  among  hospitalized  psychotics.  The  second  most 
frequent  occurrence  of  this  personality  type  is  in  the  individual  therapy 
sample. 


TABLE  21 

Percentage  of  Docile-Dependent  Personalities   (Level  II-C) 

Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

% 

of  Docile-Dependent 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 

207 

11 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 

46 

20 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 

101 

11 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

38 

18 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

3 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

56 

13 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

70 

3 

Medical  Control  Patients 

41 

5 

Ulcer  Patients 

42 

2 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

6 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

100 

2 

Total  781 

Therapeutic  Handling  of  the  Phobic  Personality 

Some  phobics  express  underlying  counterphobic  power;  others 
show  sweet  hysterical  conventionality  in  their  "preconscious"  expres- 
sions. Entirely  different  therapeutic  results  may  be  expected  from 
these  varied  multilevel  patterns  even  though  they  are  all  presented 
overtly  in  the  same  way.  Generalizations  about  the  phobic  personality 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  DOCILITY  301 

must  therefore  be  considerably  limited,  although  it  is  possible  to  ex- 
amine some  of  the  implications  of  the  docile  fagade  which  seems  to 
characterize  most  phobic  patients. 

The  first  characteristic  worth  noting  is  their  apparent  readiness  and 
eagerness  for  psychiatric  help.  The  word  help  is  used  here  in  contrast 
to  the  word  treatment — for  many  phobics  are  not  at  all  eager  for  ex- 
tended therapy.  Because  of  their  dependence,  their  admission  of  fear 
and  weakness,  they  give  the  appearance  of  being  highly  motivated  and 
cooperative  patients.  This  appearance  is  often  misleading. 

Phobic  patients  in  their  underlying  levels  have  their  share  of  all  the 
sixteen  generic  interpersonal  motivations.  They  have  as  much  (or 
perhaps  more)  ambivalence  and  conflict  as  any  other  overt  personality 
type.  The  deceptive  factor  here  is  the  fact  that  they  have  a  fa9ade  of 
cooperative,  passive  docility.  This  often  lulls  the  imperceptive  clini- 
cian into  the  expectation  that  the  patient  is  wholeheartedly  involved 
in  the  treatment  plan.  If  questioned,  the  phobic  may  appear  to  be  in 
complete  conformity  with  the  program  outhned  by  the  clinician. 
Here  we  think  of  the  typical  and  familiar  phrase,  "I'll  do  anything 
you  suggest,  Doctor." 

Thus  the  ambivalence  and  conflicting  motivation  which  we  expect 
in  almost  every  patient  tends  to  be  easily  overlooked  in  the  case  of  the 
docile  phobic. 

In  dealing  with  most  other  overt  personality  types,  the  intake  diag- 
nostician is  automatically  led  to  look  for  ambivalence  or  conflict.  In 
the  case  of  the  schizoid  patient  some  partial  abandonment  of  his  dis- 
trustful operations  is  required  in  order  for  him  to  express  the  collabora- 
tive feelings  involved  in  a  commitment  to  therapy.  Similarly,  a  strong, 
self-confident  counterphobic  patient  must  make  some  admission  of 
weakness  and  need-for-help  if  he  is  to  commit  himself  to  treatment. 
In  the  case  of  the  docile  phobic  patient,  ambivalence  is  often  present 
but  can  be  easily  overlooked  because  of  the  superficial  eagerness  of 
these  patients  to  please  and  conform  to  the  clinician's  suggestions.  For 
this  reason  these  patients  tend  to  present  tricky  and  confusing  prog- 
nostic problems.  In  the  early  days  of  the  clinical  training  program  at 
the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic,  phobic  patients  were  often  assigned  for 
therapy  to  novitiate  interns.  The  reasoning  was:  "These  patients  are 
anxious,  cooperative,  well-motivated,  and  not  too  distrustful,  and  are 
thus  excellent  patients  for  the  beginning  therapist." 

This  generalization  has  proved  optimistic.  We  have  found  it  to  be 
difficult  to  predict  the  clinical  course  of  an  overtly  docile  patient. 

In  a  preceding  section  it  has  been  pointed  out  that  phobics  on  the 
diagnostic  continuum  fall  between  hysterics  and  obsessives.  Func- 
tionally, this  means  that  they  tend  to  combine  punitive  self-deprecia- 


302 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


tion  and  bland  naivete.  The  repressive  hysterical  element  often  leads 
phobics  to  an  early  departure  from  the  clinic  as  soon  as  they  sense  that 
therapy  is  not  a  magical  cure  but  rather  a  process  of  realistic  self- 
evaluation.  The  latter,  of  course,  is  quite  alien  to  the  repressive  opera- 
tions. 

We  have  found  that  many  phobics  can  present  an  initial  facade 
which  involves  severe  anxiety,  marked  conformity  to  treatment  plans, 
and  apparent  motivation  for  therapy.  The  underlying  motivation  may 
involve  other  interpersonal  operations  and  might  predict  an  early 
"repressing"  out  of  therapy,  or  the  development  of  severe  feelings  of 
distrust  and  isolation,  etc.  Phobics  may  often  resist  (in  a  conciliatory 
manner)  the  clinician's  attempt  to  clarify  their  motivation — particu- 
larly if  they  sense  that  a  reproach  or  criticism  is  impUed.  This  is  gen- 
erally followed  by  a  reaffirmation  of  their  willingness  to  conform  to 
the  "doctor's  orders."  A  supportive  and  sympathetic  explanation  of 
the  phobic's  motivation  will  often  allow  the  prtient  to  express  his  un- 
derlying doubts,  or  fears,  or  critical  resistance  to  psychotherapy. 

Again  it  must  be  recalled  that  these  comments  are  limited  by  the 
multilevel  variations  which  differentiate  the  2,048  types  who  present 
overtly  as  docile-dependent.  The  temporal  sequence  of  interpersonal 
behavior  to  be  expected  varies  according  to  the  configuration  of  the 
total  personality.  The  therapeutic  handling  of  overt  phobics,  there- 
fore, varies  according  to  these  differences. 

References 

1.  Fenichel,  O.  The  psychoanalytic  theory  of  neurosis.  New  York:  Norton,  1945. 

2.  Freud,  S.   Analysis  of  a  phobia  in  a  five-year-old  boy.   Collected  papers.   Vol.  3. 
London:  Hogarth  Press,  1948. 

3.  Malamud,  W.  The  psychoneuroses.  In  J.  McV.  Hunt  (ed.),  Personality  and  the 
behavior  disorders.  New  York:  The  Ronald  Press  Co.,  1944. 


18 

Adjustment  Through  Cooperation. 
The  Overconventional  Personality 


Conventional,  friendly  affiliation  with  others  is  the  mode  of  adjust- 
ment discussed  in  this  chapter.  This  is  the  "77"  personality  type.  We 
shall  be  discussing  those  individuals  whose  overt  security  operations 
involve  agreeability,  and  who  strive  to  be  liked  and  accepted  by  others. 

Adaptive  Forms  of  the  Conventional  Personality 

Extroverted  friendliness  is  the  adaptive  form  of  "this  generic  secur- 
ity operation.  The  individuals  who  utilize  these  interpersonal  reflexes 
seem  to  be  comfortable  when  they  are  evoking  "good  feelings"  and 
establishing  harmonious,  amicable  relations  with  others. 

They  tend  to  seek  satisfaction  in  sociabihty  with  others.  Accepted 
values  are  important  to  them.  They  are  more  likely  to  cooperate,  to 
go  along  with  the  conventional  pattern,  to  compromise.  External 
harmony  is  more  important  than  internal  values.  They  are  less  likely 
to  emphasize  a  unique,  original,  or  highly  controversial  point  of  view. 

Individuals  who  employ  this  interpersonal  machinery  with  flexibil- 
ity are  productive  and  valuable  members  of  society.  They  are  popu- 
lar, well-liked,  and  agreeable  members  of  any  group.  They  deal  with 
social  anxiety  by  friendly,  amicable  responses. 

This  mode  of  adjustment  is  probably  the  highest  stated  ideal  of  our 
Western  civilization.  The  loving,  peaceable,  brotherly  person  is  given 
the  most  honored  role  in  the  ethical  hierarchy.  This  is,  it  must  be 
noted,  a  cultural  ideal.  The  personal  ideal  of  most  individuals  (as 
measured  by  the  interpersonal  system)  clearly  emphasizes  a  combi- 
nation of  conventionality  and  strength.  The  pure  loving  person  is  the 
third  most  idealized  figure,  power  and  sympathetic  responsibility 
being  the  ego-ideal  values  preferred  by  the  individuals  studied.  The 
person  who  acts  or  describes  himself  in  terms  of  cooperativeness  and 

303 


304  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

friendliness  seems  to  be  attempting  to  meet  the  stated  rather  than  the 
real  cultural  conventional  standard. 

Maladaptive  Forms  of  the  Conventiojial  Personality 

Individuals  who  rigidly  and  inappropriately  express  agreeable, 
afEliative  behavior  are  diagnosed  as  overconventional  personalities. 
These  are  the  persons  who  cannot  tolerate  any  critical  or  strong  or 
guilty  behavior  in  themselves.  They  continually  strive  to  please,  to 
be  accepted,  to  establish  positive  relations  with  others. 

It  is  difficult  to  describe  these  security  operations  because  the  Eng- 
lish language  has  a  scarcity  of  words  denoting  this  condition.  It  was 
pointed  out  in  Chapter  2  that  our  English  dictionaries  do  not  contain 
terms  defining  the  state  of  being  overaffectionate  or  too  friendly  or 
overcooperative.  The  notion  that  a  person  can  be  maladaptively  sweet 
is  apparently  alien  to  our  culture. 

Thus  we  face  the  dilemma  of  describing  persons  for  whom  there 
exists  no  ready-made,  common  terminology.  We  have  had  to  meet 
this  problem  (in  our  empirical  studies)  by  hyphenated  words  or  ex- 
tended phrases  denoting  the  person  who  is  abnormally  and  rigidly 
friendly. 

The  items  on  the  interpersonal  check  list  which  designate  this  con- 
dition are: 

L  M 

too  easily  influenced  by  friends  fond  of  everyone 

will  confide  in  anyone  likes  everyone 

wants  everyone's  love  too  friendly 

agrees  with  everyone  loves  everyone 

The  interpersonal  behaviors  which  diagnose  these  security  opera- 
tions involve  the  compulsive,  repetitious  expression  of  affiliative  be- 
havior. These  individuals  smile,  agree,  collaborate,  conciliate.  They 
are  extroverted  and  outgoing  to  an  intense  degree.  They  are  so  com- 
mitted to  conventional  responses  that  they  forfeit  originality  and  in- 
dividuality. 

External  values  and  approval  from  others  dominate  their  social  in- 
tercourse. Bland,  often  naive,  uninsightful  behavior  is  the  inevitable 
correlate.  Gross  misperceptions  of  social  reality  characterize  their 
approach.  They  just  cannot  see  hostility  or  power  in  themselves. 
They  avoid  feelings  of  depression.  A  rigid  overoptimism  is  quire 
typical.  They  often  misperceive  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  others 
and  tend  to  saturate  all  their  social  exchanges  with  affihative  motifs. 

The  maladaptive  aspects  of  these  security  operations  are  obvious. 
Like  any  set  of  crippled  reflexes,  the  repetitious  and  inappropriate  ex- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COOPERATION  305 

pression  of  positive  feelings  lends  to  a  general  restriction  of  personality. 
Their  repertoire  of  responses  is  narrow.  They  are  limited  only  to  the 
conventional  actions  and  perceptions.  They  forget  or  misinterpret 
other  kinds  of  behavior.  They  seem  to  fear  being  individuals.  Their 
imagination  and  creativity  is  lost  in  the  attempt  to  be  acceptable  or 
to  be  liked.  A  sterile  conventionality  or  a  self-satisfied  piousness  re- 
sults. 

The  Purpose  of  Overconventional  Behavior 

The  security  operations  of  conventional  agreeability  are  employed 
to  ward  off  anxiety.  These  individuals  are  uncomfortable  in  the 
presence  of  hostile,  unhappy,  or  power-oriented  feelings.  They  avoid 
these  responses. 

The  overconventional  person  apparently  has  learned  that  he  can 
reduce  anxiety  and  gain  heightened  self-esteem  by  means  of  opti- 
mistic blandness.  He  has  discovered  that  acceptance  and  approval 
from  others  can  be  won  by  means  of  friendly  operations.  He  feels 
safe,  comfortable,  secure  when  he  is  employing  these  protections. 

When  the  reality  situation  involves  unconventional  behavior  or 
threatens  their  optimistic  (and  often  shallow)  approach,  these  sub- 
jects become  upset.  They  may  strive  to  handle  the  situation  by  in- 
creased optimism  and  sociability.  If  these  maneuvers  fail,  they  tend  to 
get  out  of  the  anxiety-provoking  field.  Psychiatric  evaluation  and 
self-exploration  are,  of  course,  among  the  most  threatening  events 
faced  by  this  personality  type. 

Their  unique  methods  for  handling  the  anxiety  aroused  by  psychia- 
tric referral  will  be  discussed  below. 

The  Effect  of  Overconventional  Behavior 

Friendly  agreeability  tends  to  provoke  approval  and  friendliness 
from  others.  In  the  systematic  language,  LM  pulls  MN  from  others. 
The  extroverted,  optimistic  person  trains  others  to  like  him.  Co- 
operativeness  induces  a  reciprocal  positive  response  in  others. 

These  reciprocal  relations  are,  of  course,  part  of  the  folklore  of  our 
culture.  The  Dale  Carnegie  texts  and  the  salesmen's  manuals  have 
pointed  out  the  effect  of  the  "positive  approach,"  and  our  empirical 
studies  have  tended  to  confirm  these  bromides. 

A  qualifying  remark  must  accompany  these  generalizations.  The 
principle  of  reciprocal  interpersonal  relations  is  a  probability  state- 
ment. It  tends  to  hold  most  of  the  time.  There  are  many  cases  where 
it  does  not  work.  LM  does  not  always  pull  MN. 

The  maladaptive  intensity  of  the  response  provides  a  special  case. 
Many  situations  call  for  anger  or  sorrow  or  power.   If  the  overcon- 


3o6  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

ventional  person  is  unable  to  respond  appropriately,  his  attempts  to 
win  approval  may  fail. 

The  personality  of  the  "other  one"  is  another  important  factor. 
If  the  alter  in  any  social  interaction  tends  to  respond  with  a  different 
inteq^ersonal  reflex,  then  the  ability  of  the  overconventional  person 
to  pull  approval  is  limited.  Skeptical  individuals  can  be  infuriated  by 
overoptimism  in  another.  Power-oriented  individuals  may  see  co- 
operative agreeability  as  a  form  of  docility  and  an  invitation  for  them 
to  increase  their  bossy  reflexes. 

The  selectivity  of  interpersonal  relationships  enters  the  picture  at 
this  point.  Overconventional  people  tend  to  avoid  persons  and  places 
which  threaten  their  fa9ades.  The  "sicker"  or  more  restricted  the 
person  is,  the  less  able  he  is  to  tolerate  differences  which  raise  anxiety. 

Thus  it  often  transpires  that  bland  overfriendly  persons  tend  to 
gravitate  towards  other  agreeable,  optimistic,  pious,  conventional  peo- 
ple and  do  not  tend  to  seek  out  antisocial  or  highly  original  partners. 

In  cases  where  negative  feeUngs  are  involved,  these  patients  charac- 
teristically resolve  the  situation  by  the  maneuver  of  "going  along" 
with  the  feelings  of  their  in-group.  If  the  group  to  which  they  con- 
form is  angry,  they  can  be  angry;  but  the  hostihty  is  directed  against 
an  out-group  figure  and  it  is  usually  not  expressed  directly.  These  sub- 
jects can  be  very  critical  of  an  out-group  person  who  is  not  present. 

The  generalizations  made  in  this  section  require  qualification. 
Multilevel  variations  and  conflicts  provide  new  complications.  Some 
patients  with  overconventional  fagades  tend  to  have  underlying  feel- 
ings which  involve  less-conventional  themes  (such  as  masochism  or 
sadism).  These  "preconscious"  tendencies  may  lead  them  to  become 
involved  with  individuals  who  are  unloving  and  unconventional. 

Even  with  the  qualifications  introduced  by  multilevel  conflicts 
and  by  the  personality  of  the  "other  one"  the  general  principle  of 
reciprocity  holds  as  a  low-order  probability  statement.  Patients  with 
low  scores  on  nonconformity  (i.e.,  the  F  scale  on  the  MMPI)  see  them- 
selves as  loving  and  cooperative  and  are  seen  in  the  same  positive 
fashion  by  fellow  group  therapy  members. 

Clinical  Manifestations  of  the  "77"  Personality 

There  are  several  cHnical  characteristics  of  the  overconventional 
personahty. 

First  it  should  be  noted  that  this  personality  type  is  not  a  common 
visitor  to  the  psychiatric  clinic.  The  essence  of  the  psychiatric  process 
is  self-examination  and  an  analysis  of  one's  own  unique  patterns  of 
living.    The  essence  of  the  bland,  friendly  overagreeable  mode  of 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COOPERATION  307 

adjustment  is  the  inhibition  of  one's  uniqueness  and  one's  individual 
feelings  and  the  emphasis  on  external  values. 

The  overconventional  person  does  not  come  to  the  psychiatric 
clinic  because  of  a  dissatisfaction  with  self  or  a  desire  for  self-exami- 
nation. He  is  not  depressed.  He  does  not  complain  of  internalized 
emotional  problems  (e.g.,  guilt,  distrust). 

The  specific  symptomatic  picture  can  vary,  but  the  interpersonal 
message  of  bland  self-acceptance  is  usually  present. 

There  are  three  reasons  which  bring  these  patients  to  a  psychiatric 
clinic.  These  are  (1)  generalized  "nervousness"  or  anxiety,  vaguely 
defined  and  not  tied  to  emotional  causes;  (2)  physical  symptoms,  often 
with  a  direct  symboUc  meaning;  (3)  complaints  about  the  behavior  of 
others. 

The  first  of  these  complaints — generalized  nonspecific  anxiety — is 
the  most  common  symptom,  so  typical  that  it  is  quite  diagnostic. 
These  patients  use  the  words  tension,  nervousness,  and  cnixiety  in  their 
self-descriptions.  The  significance  of  these  particular  terms  is  that 
they  have  a  relatively  vague  quality.  They  designate  a  symptom 
which  is  not  tied  to  a  specific,  recognized  emotional  problem.  The 
patient  does  not  know  why  he  is  anxious.  He  is  not  depressed  or  fear- 
ful. This  differentiates  the  overconventional  from  the  phobic  personal- 
ity. The  latter  is  unhappy  and  sees  himself  as  weak  and  timid.  The 
overconventional  person  comes  to  the  clinic  because  of  anxiety  which 
is  described  as  a  phenomenon  quite  removed  from  his  personality. 

This  symptom  of  vague  tension  can  generally  be  traced  to  an  inter- 
personal trauma  or  friction  in  the  patient's  life.  Pressure  is  being  put 
on  the  patient  to  react  in  a  negative  way  (hostile  or  defeated).  The 
overconventional  person  cannot  handle  these  situations  appropriately. 
He  strives  not  to  recognize  the  emotions  which  they  arouse  in  him. 
His  rigid  attempts  to  misperceive  and  deny  negative  feelings  in  him- 
self and  others  seal  off  the  emotional  meaning  and  leave  him  only 
with  intense  anxiety.  The  threat  of  his  own  negative  feelings  (usually 
provoked  by  the  traumatic  external  pressure)  is  the  most  intolerable 
experience  for  this  personality  type.  He  comes  to  the  clinic,  needless 
to  say,  not  consciously  desiring  to  have  the  cover  removed  from  his 
misperceptions  and  negative  emotions  but  to  have  the  anxiety  removed. 
When  these  patients  sense  that  psychotherapy  might  threaten  their 
bland  denial  they  clearly  express  their  disinterest  in  treatment. 

These  patients  present  particularly  pathetic  pictures  when  they 
arrive  at  the  psychiatric  clinic.  Their  fear  of  their  own  negative  feel- 
ing brings  on  the  tension,  but  psychiatric  interviews  tend  to  arouse 
exactly  the  same  anxiety.  Caught  between  the  pain  of  the  illness  and 


3o8  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

the  pain  of  the  cure,  they  usually  handle  this  dilemma  by  intensifica- 
tion of  their  favored  security  operations;  that  is  to  say,  they  attempt 
to  re-establish  their  bland,  optimistic  protections  and  move  themselves 
out  of  the  therapeutic  situation. 

The  complaint  of  diffuse  tension  can  thus  be  seen  as  a  symptom 
external  to  the  patient's  view  of  his  own  character  structure.  They 
come  to  the  clinic  seeking  relief  from  this  isolated  symptom  and  not 
psychological  explanation.  This  extended  discussion  of  one  sympto- 
matic presentation  has  been  outlined  in  detail  for  two  reasons.  First, 
it  is  important  for  the  clinician  to  recognize  the  fact  that  the  com- 
plaint of  tension  or  nervousness  is  not  attached  to  the  patient's  con- 
ception of  himself  as  a  person.  The  intensity  of  the  anxiety  may  make 
these  patients  appear  to  be  well  motivated  for  therapy.  The  bland, 
conventional  nature  of  their  security  can  be  revealed  by  sensitive  inter- 
viewing and  is  picked  up  very  clearly  in  the  Level  I  and  Level  II  tests. 
Failure  to  distinguish  this  difference  may  lead  to  a  breakdown  in 
communication  and  the  patient's  flight  from  the  clinic.  The  second 
important  aspect  of  this  syndrome  is  its  frequency.  Over  50  per  cent 
of  the  overconventional  patients  seen  in  the  Kaiser  Foundation  men- 
tion the  vague,  nonspecific  terms  tension,  nervousness,  or  anxiety  in 
describing  their  reasons  for  coming  to  the  clinic. 

The  second  most  frequent  symptom  mentioned  by  "77"  patients 
involves  physical  complaints.  These  are  often  symbolic  of  unrecog- 
nized emotional  conflicts.  Headaches  (which  our  clinicians  believe 
to  reflect  underlying  hostility)  and  menstrual  complaints  (believed  to 
reflect  sexual  constriction)  are  probably  the  most  common  physical 
symptoms.  Examination  of  the  case  material  of  overconventional  pa- 
tients reveals  that  the  great  majority  of  the  female  patients  are  sexu- 
ally frigid.  This  is  sometimes  recognized  but  is  rarely  developed  as  a 
complaint,  these  patients  being  unalarmed  about  this  condition.  The 
physical  symptoms  classically  characteristic  of  hysterical  blandness 
(e.g.,  paralyses,  amnesias,  anaesthesias)  are  rarely  seen  in  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  clinic.  When  they  do  appear  they  are  not  generally  re- 
ported by  the  pure  overconventional  personality  (77)  but  by  severely 
or  chronically  disturbed  patients  with  conflicted  fagades.  We  think 
here  of  the  schizoid-hysteric  (47)  or  the  masochistic-overconven- 
tional  conflict  (57). 

The  third  clinical  characteristic  of  the  "77"  personality  involves 
complaints  about  the  behavior  of  other  people.  A  sudden  flare-up 
of  marital  trouble  (previously  unrecognized)  is  a  common  precipitat- 
ing event.  The  spouse  may  demand  a  divorce,  thus  breaking  through 
the  optimistic  fa9ade  and  confronting  the  "surprised"  patient  with 
unpleasant  emotions.  Antisocial  behavior  on  the  part  of  a  family  mem- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COOPERATION  309 

ber  (delinquency,  crime,  sexual  eccentricity)  may  bring  about  the 
same  result. 

These  situations  confront  the  patient  with  emotions  which  his 
security  operations  have  previously  denied.  It  must  be  noted,  how- 
ever, that  the  resulting  anxiety  is  not  seen  as  intrinsic  or  related  to  the 
personality  but  (like  the  diffuse  tension  or  physical  symptom)  is  seen 
as  external. 

Turning  from  the  symptomatic  picture  to  the  clinical  impression 
given  by  these  patients,  we  see  a  new  set  of  diagnostic  cues. 

The  bland  overconventional  person  is  often  seen  as  immature  by  the 
clinician.  This  term  runs  through  the  typical  case  reports  and  reflects 
the  naivete,  the  artless,  childlike  ingenuousness  which  these  security 
operations  maintain.  These  patients  see  no  evil,  hear  no  evil,  think  no 
evil,  do  no  evil.  They  handle  interpersonal  situations  by  complaisant, 
serene  machinery. 

The  rigidity  by  means  of  which  these  individuals  can  distort  and 
misinterpret  reality  can  reach  astounding  proportions.  These  misper- 
ceptions  (sincere  and  not  deliberate)  can  lead  to  disastrous  misunder- 
standings. We  think  here  of  the  patient  who  employed  two  solid  layers 
of  bland  optimistic  friendliness  to  handle  feelings  of  despair  so  severe 
as  to  reach  psychotic  proportions.  In  the  face  of  several  catastrophic 
failures  (loss  of  two  jobs,  threatened  divorce),  this  patient  insisted  in 
the  intake  interview  that  everything  was  going  well,  that  he  was  not 
depressed,  etc. 

The  discrepancy  between  the  reality  situation  and  his  happy  re- 
actions finally  emerged.  The  intake  worker  reviewed  with  the  patient 
the  intense  conflict  between  desperate  fearful  depression  and  the 
cheerful  fagade.  The  latter  operations  were  supported,  but  the  need 
for  treatment  was  stressed.  The  patient  was  delighted  with  the  course 
of  the  interview,  enthusiastically  accepted  the  mild  summary  of  the 
clinician,  and  eagerly  cooperated  in  making  plans  for  therapy,  arrang- 
ing future  appointments,  etc. 

Within  two  days  the  clinician  received  phone  calls  from  three  irate 
and  puzzled  people  (his  wife,  his  employer,  and  the  referring  phy- 
sician), all  of  whom  had  been  informed  by  the  patient  that  "the  psy- 
chiatrist said  I  am  perfectly  normal  and  don't  need  treatment."  In  a 
subsequent  interview  the  patient  remembered  the  negative  or  reality 
side  of  the  clinician's  original  summary  and  stated  that  he  had  "for- 
gotten" the  plans  for  therapy  and  discovered  the  appointment  slip 
which  had  been  "lost"  in  his  wallet. 

This  patient  was  not  a  dishonest  or  prevaricating  person.  The 
rigidity  and  intensity  of  the  ingenuous  naivete,  as  well  as  the  com- 
plete crippling  of  any  other  interpersonal  reflexes,  were  quite  evident 


3IO  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

in  his  Level  I  and  II  test  patterns  and  testified  eloquently  to  the  pres- 
ence of  a  blanket  denial  process  which  made  it  intolerable  for  this 
person  to  face  unpleasant  reality. 

This  same  process  is  regularly  observed  in  group  therapy  where 
hysterical  patients  completely  misperceive  hostility  in  others  and  for- 
get the  occasion  when  they  have  been  momentarily  angry  or  depressed 
in  the  group. 

The  Relationship  of  Over  conventionality  to 
Standard  Psychiatric  Diagnosis 

Patients  who  manifest  the  reflexes  of  intense,  maladaptive  over- 
conventionality  are  often  given  the  psychiatric  diagnosis  of  hysterics. 

If  we  review  the  clinical  characteristics  of  the  conventional  per- 
sonality, we  will  observe  that  they  tend  to  fit  the  general  conception 
of  hysterical  behavior — the  physical  symptoms,  bland  denial  of  emo- 
tional problems,  etc. 

The  chapters  in  psychiatric  texts  which  describe  the  hysteric  usu- 
ally center  the  discussion  around  the  dramatic  symptomology:  fugues, 
amnesias,  paralyses,  etc.  (3,2)  In  recent  years  these  colorful  symptoms 
tend  to  appear  in  diminishing  frequency  and  the  diagnosis  of  hysteria 
is  increasingly  being  based  on  dynamic,  interpersonal  or  psychosexual 
criteria  (4,  1). 

The  bland,  optimistic  conventionality  of  the  hysteric  has  been  dis- 
cussed in  the  literature  for  over  sixty  years.  Charcot  defined  this  diag- 
nostic characteristic  in  describing  "la  belle  indifference  des  hyster- 
iques." 

The  current  trend  in  diagnosis  seems  to  emphasize  the  dynamic 
aspects  of  the  hysterical  personality.  Schafer  in  his  competent  diag- 
nostic volume  (4)  consistently  employs  "functional"  or  dynamic 
variables  rather  than  symptomatic  or  descriptive  cues.  He  defines 
hysterics  as  persons  who  "rigidly  and  persuasively  resort  to  the  defense 
of  repression."  He  speaks  of  the  narrowed  cultural  and  intellectual 
interests,  the  impaired  ability  to  think  independently  or  to  express 
original,  individual  themes.  He  also  refers  to  the  naivete  of  these  pa- 
tients. 

In  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  system  the  term  hysterical  per- 
sonality is  used  to  describe  patients  whose  presenting  operations  stress 
bland,  narrow  conventionality.  It  must  be  emphasized  that  we  are 
diagnosing  overt  operations.  Hysterics  vary  considerably  in  their  un- 
derlying motivation.  Some  patients  employ  an  optimistic  overcoop- 
erative  fagade  to  mask  underlying  schizoid  or  sadistic  feelings.  Other 
overt  hysterics  present  solid,  four-layer  structures  of  friendly  con- 
geniality. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COOPERATION  3 1 1 

When  we  employ  the  term  hysteric,  we  do  not  necessarily  desig- 
nate the  "simple-hysteric-serving-girl"  syndrome  for  which  sugges- 
tion and  hypnosis  have  traditionally  been  used  as  therapeutic  tools. 
Patients  with  overt  hysterical  operations  can  be  very  complex  in  their 
multilevel  patterns.  A  wide  variety  of  treatment  regimes  can  be 
recommended  depending  on  the  nature  of  the  underlying  material, 
the  ability  to  tolerate  the  warded-off  emotions,  etc. 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  "77"  Personality 

Here  is  a  summary  of  the  empirical  studies  of  the  overconventional 
personality. 

1 .  Patients  who  utilize  overconventional  security  operations  present 
MMPI  profiles  which  stress  hysteria  (Hy)  and  denial-of-psycho- 
pathology  (K)  and  which  underemphasize  schizoid  isolation  (Sc) 
and  nonconformity  (F). 

2.  These  patients  are  not  well  motivated  for  psychotherapy.  They 
remain  in  treatment  for  an  average  of  nine  sessions  and  rank  fifth 
among  the  eight  diagnostic  types  on  this  variable. 

3.  They  are  closely  identified  (consciously)  with  their  mothers  and 
their  marital  partners — ranking  third  among  the  eight  diagnostic  types 
on  this  variable. 

4.  They  are  the  most  consciously  identified  with  their  fathers  of 
any  diagnostic  group. 

5.  They  misperceive  the  behavior  of  others  by  attributing  too 
much  friendliness  and  affiliativeness  to  others.  They  tend  to  blanket 
others  (in  their  therapy  groups)  with  the  same  conventional  sweetness 
that  they  claim  for  themselves. 

6.  The  overconventional  personality  appears  in  certain  cultural 
and  institutional  samples  more  frequently  than  others.  The  percentage 
of  hysterics  in  various  samples  at  Level  I-M  is  presented  in  Table  22. 

The  highest  percentage  of  overconventional  cases  is  presented  by 
the  self-inflicted  dermatitis  group.  These  patients  bend  over  back- 
wards to  inhibit  the  unconventional  at  Level  I,  although  in  their  fan- 
tasies they  are  more  hostile  than  any  other  sample  (see  Chapter  24). 

Another  high  percentage  of  hysterical  subjects  is  found  in  the  hy- 
pertensive example.  This  is  an  expected  result.  It  has  been  repeatedly 
claimed  in  the  psychosomatic  literature  that  hypertensives  present  un- 
usually sweet  friendly  fagades.  This  bit  of  clinical  folklore  has  been 
confirmed  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  studies  on  psychoso- 
matic subjects,  in  which  we  have  found  hypertensives  presenting  the 
facades  of  conventionality. 

An  equally  high  percentage  of  hysterical  subjects  is  found  in  the 
individual  therapy  sample.  We  have  already  noted  that  they  do  not 


537 

9 

415 

3 

133 

8 

121 

10 

31 

6 

57 

14 

71 

11 

109 

6 

49 

12 

49 

12 

43 

10 

37 

5 

93 

8 

39 

0 

52 

10 

28 

4 

39 

3 

312  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

TABLE  22 

Percentage  of  Cooperative-Overconventional  Personalities  (Level  I-M) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Cooperative-Overconventional 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
College  Undergraduates 
University  Psychiatric  Clinic 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Ulcer  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patients 
University  Counseling  Center 
University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 
Stockade  Prisoners  (Male) 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients 
Officers  in  Military  Service 

Total  1903 

tend  to  remain  long  in  treatment.  The  identity  of  the  groups  with  the 
next  highest  frequency  of  hysterical  personalities  is,  however,  some- 
what startling.  The  ulcer  sample,  the  obesity  sample,  and  the  stockade 
prisoner  sample  are  tied  for  third  rank.  This  suggests  that  the  most 
conventional  sample  (middle  class  women  seen  in  a  nonpsychiatric 
setting)  and  the  most  antisocial  sample  (prisoners)  share  the  same 
percentage  of  naive,  bland  subjects! 

From  the  standpoint  of  a  multilevel  theory,  the  latter  result  is  not 
completely  unexpected.  Many  of  the  most  severely  antisocial  indi- 
viduals present  fa9ades  of  piety  and  virtue  which  are  almost  painful  to 
observe.  Alany  delinquents,  addicts,  criminals  are  characterized  by  a 
bland,  naive,  innocent  front  behind  which  rage  intense  feelings  of  dis- 
trust or  rebellion. 

The  same  is  true  of  some  institutionalized  psychotics.  This  fact  has 
confused  some  psychologists  who  observed  that  some  of  the  most  se- 
verely disturbed  paranoids  and  deluded  schizophrenics  presented  hys- 
terical, repressive  MMPI  profiles.  Some  state-hospital  psychologists 
have  reported  that  the  MMPI  is  invalid  because  so  many  psychotic 
patients  have  conventional,  bland  records.  This  objection  completely 
misses  the  multilevel  complexity  of  personality.  Many  hospitalized 
patients  are  psychotic  because  they  tried  to  maintain  a  brittle  fa9ade  of 
pious,  self-satisfied  virtue  and  were  unable  to  tolerate  their  own  intense 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COOPERATION  3 1 3 

hostile  or  guilty  feelings.  Many  psychotics  and  delinquents  attempt 
to  preserve  the  appearance  of  naive  innocence  and  are  diagnosed  (by 
the  interpersonal  system)  as  hysterics  or  hypernormals  (at  Levels  I 
and  II)  with  underlying  feelings  of  a  more  antisocial  nature. 

7.  The  frequency  of  this  personality  type  at  Level  II-C  is  slightly 
different  from  the  picture  at  Level  I-M  (see  Table  23).  The  sample 
of  normal  controls  (labeled  medical  controls)  manifests  the  largest 
percentage  of  overconventional  personalities.  Individual  therapy  pa- 
tients again  provide  a  larger  percentage  of  this  type.  The  psycho- 
somatic and  neurotic  samples  run  about  equal  to  chance  expectancy 
except  for  the  ulcer  group.  The  self-diagnosis  of  this  latter  sympto- 
matic sample  seems  to  emphasize  stronger  and  more  competitive  feel- 
ings (see  Chapter  24). 

TABLE  23 

Percentage  of  Cooperative-Overconventional   Personalities  (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Cooperative-Overconventional 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermautis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
iMedical  Control  Patients 
Ulcer  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

Total  781 

8.  Hysteric  patients  consciously  perceive  their  parents  to  be  sweet 
and  docile.  They  describe  their  fathers  as  being  conventional  and 
loving  (ranked  third  out  of  the  eight  diagnostic  groups  on  the  LM 
axis).  They  see  their  mothers  as  being  sweet,  docile,  and  trustful 
(ranked  second  on  the  affiliative  axis  and  third  on  the  passivity  axis). 

9.  These  patients,  on  the  contrary,  see  their  spouses  as  relatively 
hostile  (ranked  fifth  on  the  affihative  axis).  They  describe  their 
marital  partners  as  more  hostile  than  do  the  schizoid  and  narcissistic 
patients.  This  seems  to  fit  in  with  the  clinical  finding  that  these  pa- 
tients come  to  the  chnic  not  because  of  dissatisfaction  with  their  own 
character  structure  or  with  their  past  life  (e.g.,  their  parents)  but 
because  of  current  external  stress  (which  often  involves  misbehavior 
of  or  rejection  by  their  spouses) .  The  hysterics  thus  "cross  the  circle," 


207 

15 

46 

9 

101 

10 

38 

18 

31 

19 

56 

14 

70 

20 

41 

22 

42 

2 

49 

12 

100 

11 

314  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

attributing  themes  to  their  marital  partners  which  are  the  opposite  of 
their  own  self-conceived  sweetness.  Narcissistic  patients,  it  might  be 
noted,  do  the  opposite.  They  present  themselves  as  superior,  snobbish, 
competitive,  and  somewhat  exploitive.  They  picture  their  spouses  as 
being  the  most  naive,  docile,  and  gullible. 

References 

1.  Fenichel,  O.  The  psychoanalytic  theory  of  neurosis.  New  York:  Norton,  1945. 

2.  Landis,  C,  and  M.  Marjorie  Bolles.    Text  book  of  abnormal  psychology   (rev. 
ed.).  New  York:  The  Macmillan  Co.,  1950. 

3.  Malamud,  VV.  The  psychoneuroses.  In  J.  McV.  Hunt  (Ed.),  Personality  and  the 
behavior  disorders.  New  York:  The  Ronald  Press  Co.,  1944. 

4.  ScHAFER,  Roy.    The  clinical  application  of  psychological  tests:  Diagnostic  sum- 
maries and  case  studies.  New  York:  International  University  Press,  1948,  p.  34<5. 


19 

Adjustment  Through  Responsibility: 
The  Hypernormal  Personality 


In  this  chapter  we  shall  become  acquainted  with  the  responsible  hyper- 
normal personality  type.  This  comprises  those  patients  whose  overt 
behavior  locates  in  the  ON  octant  of  the  diagnostic  grid.  This  is  the 
"88"  personality  type.  These  individuals  employ  strong  and  conven- 
tional security  operations.  They  present  themselves  as  reasonable, 
successful,  sympathetic,  mature.  They  avoid  the  appearance  of  weak- 
ness or  unconventionality. 

Adaptive  Forms  of  the  Responsible  Personality  Type 

Here  we  deal  with  the  individual  who  attempts  to  present  himself 
as  a  "normal"  person.  He  presents  himself  as  strong — but  his  power 
and  self-confident  independence  are  used  in  an  affiliative  way.  He 
strives  to  be  close  to  others — to  help,  counsel,  support,  and  sympathize. 
He  wants  to  be  seen  as  tender  with  his  intimates,  reasonable  and  re- 
sponsible with  his  acquaintances. 

These  individuals  often  give  the  impression  of  maturity  and  parental 
strength.  They  appear  sound,  sympathetic,  considerate.  They  are 
often  popular  figures — they  attempt  to  get  along  well  with  others 
and  to  provoke  admiration  from  others.  They  are  leaned  upon  and 
depended  upon  by  other  people.  They  strive  to  fulfill  an  idealized 
role  of  successful  conventionality. 

Maladaptive  Forms  of  the  Hypernormal  Personality 

An  inflexible,  repetitious  use  of  responsible,  hypernormal  reflexes 
leads  to  a  maladaptive  condition.  Individuals  of  this  type  cannot  take 
a  passive  or  aggressive  or  bitter  role,  even  when  it  is  called  for.  They 
avoid  these  latter  behaviors  so  compulsively  that  they  become  carica- 
tures of  hypernormality. 

3»5 


3i6  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

These  individuals  "knock  themselves  out"  to  be  popular.  Their 
attempts  to  be  helpful  and  responsible  are  often  inappropriate.  They 
may  overextend  themselves  in  promises  to  others — offers  of  help  and 
sympathy  which  they  cannot  fulfill.  They  may  desperately  attempt 
to  maintain  the  fa9ade  of  normality  when  the  situation  and  their  own 
private  feelings  involve  other  reactions.  They  are  often  driven  by 
relentless  ideals  of  service  and  contribution  to  others. 

Extreme  NO  behavior  inevitably  leads  to  a  bland,  uninsightful 
fagade.  These  individuals  cannot  tolerate  unconventional  or  weak 
feelings.  They  are  so  compulsively  attached  to  their  hypemormal 
strivings  that  they  completely  deny  and  inhibit  feelings  of  frustra- 
tion and  passivity. 

These  extreme  operations  generally  indicate  severe  conflicts  which 
are  expressed  not  in  the  classic  symptoms  of  neurosis,  but  in  indirect 
(often  psychosomatic)  manifestations. 

The  Purpose  of  Responsible  or  Hypemormal  Behavior 

Those  human  beings  who  are  strong  and  conventionally  normal 
have  selected  these  operations  because  they  find  them  most  effective 
in  warding  off  anxiety.  Their  feeUngs  of  self-esteem  are  bolstered  by 
appearing  mature  and  generous.  They  are  most  secure  when  they  are 
involved  in  close,  friendly  protective  relationship  with  dependent 
others. 

They  are,  we  assume,  most  threatened  by  the  prospect  of  appear- 
ing defeated,  deprived,  unfriendly,  or  passive.  Their  genial,  generous 
operations  tend  to  relieve  feelings  of  anger,  helplessness,  or  isolation. 
They  give  the  assurance  (at  least  consciously)  of  being  involved  in 
tender,  protective  relations  with  others. 

There  are  many  obvious  rewards  to  the  responsible  hypemormal 
way  of  life.  This  mode  of  adjustment  is  close  to  the  cultural  ideal. 
It  thus  brings  great  conscious  superego  satisfactions.  These  patients 
are  the  most  self-satisfied  individuals  seen  in  the  clinic. 

In  its  adaptive  form  this  is  a  most  positive  and  socially  constructive 
personality  type.  In  that  Utopian  society  where  skepticism,  sternness, 
competition,  or  modesty  would  not  be  necessary  responses  and  where 
consistent  aflSliative  behavior  would  be  appropriate,  the  generous  NO 
type  would  be  the  rule.  Even  in  the  nonutopian  twentieth  century 
culture  the  ideals  of  tender,  protective  nurturance  are  undoubtedly 
the  most  appealing  standards.  The  conventionally  successful  and 
popular  person  in  our  society  is  usually  the  one  who  employs  the  NO 
interpersonal  reflexes  a  large  part  of  the  time. 

Compulsive  and  inappropriate  maintenance  of  these  operations 
leads  to  the  phenomenon  of  the  "hollow  man" — isolated  by  his  self- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  RESPONSIBILITY  317 

satisfied  piety  from  the  realities  of  life  and  (more  dangerously)  from 
his  own  inner  feelings  of  bitterness  or  weakness. 

The  Effect  of  "88"  Behavior 

Responsible,  protective  behavior  pulls  dependence  and  respect 
from  others.  The  person  who  overtly  gives  tends  to  attract  those  who 
want  to  receive.  In  the  language  of  the  interpersonal  system,  NO 
pulls  KL. 

Tender,  supportive  operations  tend  to  train  others  to  agree,  con- 
ciliate, and  depend.  This  rule  (like  the  previous  generalizations  about 
interpersonal  reciprocity)  is  a  probability  statement.  Generosity  does 
not  always  pull  friendly  dependence.  Those  who  are  rigidly  com- 
mitted to  other  interpersonal  reflexes  will  react  to  the  reasonable,  gen- 
erous person  with  their  favored  responses.  In  general,  however,  most 
people  tend  to  expect  good  things  from  those  who  promise  good 
things. 

Another  exception  to  this  rule  of  reciprocity  (i.e.,  NO  pulls  KL) 
occurs  when  the  hypernormal  behavior  is  extreme  or  inappropriate. 
Here  we  think  of  the  overmotherly  woman,  the  compulsive  popularity 
seeker,  the  overprotective  parent.  While  these  behaviors  generally 
tend  to  pull  cooperative  dependence,  their  uncalled-for  intensity  may 
eventually  provoke  resentment  or  frustration  from  the  "other  one." 

In  therapy  groups,  the  "88"  individual  takes  the  role  of  the  assistant 
therapist.  He  encourages,  suggests,  and  sympathizes  with  the  other 
patients.  He  does  not  exhibit  needy  or  helpless  reflexes  but  is  seen  as 
the  competent  helpful  leader. 

Typically  the  group  members  cannot  understand  why  he  is  in 
therapy.  They  see  his  reasonable,  generous  fagade  as  an  ideal  adjust- 
ment. At  this  point  the  interpersonal  network  tightens.  The  other 
patients  increasingly  put  more  dependent  pressure  on  the  "88"  person. 
He  is  now  being  asked  for  help,  expected  to  give,  and  is  given  less  and 
less  allowance  to  present  his  own  problems  as  a  fellow  patient. 

These  patients  are  thus  the  popularity  leaders  of  the  group.  By 
acting  in  a  hypernormal  way  they  are  not  seen  as  patients  needing 
help.  They  build  up  an  interpersonal  process  which  would  lead  to 
their  getting  no  therapeutic  help  from  group  therapy.  At  this  junc- 
ture the  task  of  the  therapist  is  to  step  in  and  assist  the  "88"  person  in 
understanding  what  he  has  done  to  the  others  to  block  himself  off 
from  the  possibility  of  help. 

Clinical  Manifestations  of  the  Hypernormal  Personality  Type 

Patients  whose  overt  security  operations  strive  towards  normality 
do  not  present  the  typical  neurotic  symptoms  when  they  appear  in 


ji8  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

the  psychiatric  clinic.  They  are  not  anxious  or  depressed.  They  do 
not  report  interpersonal  failures.  They  do  not  complain  of  timidity, 
isolation,  distrust,  etc.  They  tend  to  describe  their  emotional  adjust- 
ment as  adequate  and  normal. 

Why  then,  do  they  come  to  the  clinic?  In  the  Kaiser  Foundation 
clinic  which  services  a  large  general  hospital,  23  per  cent  of  all  appli- 
cations are  hypernormal  individuals.  The  overwhelming  majority  of 
these  patients  are  not  self-referred,  but  have  come  at  the  request  of  a 
physician.  Their  symptoms  are  psychosomatic  or  physical. 

From  40  to  50  per  cent  of  patients  with  psychosomatic  diseases  fall 
in  the  NO  octant.  (The  frequency  expected  by  chance  is  12.5  per 
cent.)  Seventy-nine  per  cent  of  psychosomatic  patients  fall  in  the  NO 
octant  or  its  two  neighboring  octants.  For  this  reason  patients  who 
locate  in  this  sector  of  the  diagnostic  grid  can  be  called  psychosomatic- 
type  personalities. 

It  must  be  pointed  out  that  we  refer  here  to  organ  neurosis  condi- 
tions and  not  to  somatic  expressions  of  anxiety  (nervous  stomach, 
transient  pains,  etc.).  The  latter  are  typical  of  the  docile  phobic  per- 
sonality. The  symptoms  manifested  by  the  "88"  personality  are  not 
transitory  expressions  of  tension.  The  hypernormal  personality  is  usu- 
ally successful  in  warding  off  anxiety  and  presents  a  bland,  strong 
fa9ade. 

In  addition  to  psychosomatic  symptoms  the  "88"  personality  often 
comes  to  the  clinic  for  the  purpose  of  putting  indirect  pressure  on 
family  members.  They  may  present  a  story  of  marital  difficulty  in 
which  it  becomes  clear  that  the  spouse  or  a  child  is  "sick  and  in  need 
of  help."  The  subject  may  recount  a  history  of  patient  tolerance  of 
the  family  member — the  implication  being  that  the  diagnostician  will 
give  the  patient  a  clean  bill  of  health  and  suggest  that  the  errant  spouse 
be  brought  in  to  treatment.  The  poised,  "mature"  reasonableness  of 
the  "88"  fagade  may  tempt  the  inexperienced  clinician  into  collaborat- 
ing in  the  plans  to  inveigle  family  members  into  therapy. 

A  third  reason  for  the  "88"  personality  coming  to  the  clinic  in- 
volves certain  forms  of  isolated  behavior  disorders  such  as  alcoholism, 
gambling,  or  certain  sexual  aberrations.  These  patients  may  be  self- 
referred  or  sent  in  by  family  pressure  or  court  order.  They  readily  see 
the  symptomatic  behavior  as  ego-alien — but  isolate  it  from  their  per- 
sonality. The  motto  for  these  patients  might  be:  "I  am  a  well-adjusted 
nice  guy — if  only  I  could  get  rid  of  that  crazy  behavior  pattern." 

Many  alcoholics  or  addicts  fall  into  other  diagnostic  categories. 
The  guilty  or  the  defiant  types  do  not,  of  course,  locate  in  the  "88" 
sector  of  the  diagnostic  grid.  Alany  behavior-disorder  patients,  how- 
ever, do  attempt  to  maintain  a  repressive  hypernormal  fa9ade. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  RESPONSIBILITY  319 

Many  cases  of  impotency  or  frigidity  fall  in  the  hypernormal  sec- 
tor. Here  again  the  symptom  (like  that  of  the  psychosomatic)  is  seen 
as  isolated  from  the  well-adjusted  personality. 

Many  severely  deluded  paranoid  patients  present  themselves  as 
hypernormal  at  the  symptomatic  level.  This  is  really  not  a  paradoxical 
situation  if  the  theory  of  levels  is  kept  in  mind. 

Level  I  summarizes  the  patient's  impact  on  the  clinician.  The  es- 
sence of  certain  forms  of  many  severe  paranoid  conditions  is  that  the 
patient  strives  to  appear  hypernormal.  When  we  assign  this  Level  I 
diagnosis,  we  do  not  assume  that  this  means  the  patient  really  is 
normal,  but  rather  that  his  security  operations  at  this  level  strive  to 
create  this  impression. 

Very  often  patients  reporting  to  an  out-patient  clinic  after  hospital- 
ization for  a  psychotic  break  present  as  hypernormal  at  the  level  of 
symptoms.  These  patients  have  utilized  repressive  measures  to  handle 
their  psychotic  impulses.  They  are  sitting  on  their  conflicts  and  striv- 
ing to  maintain  a  fa9ade  of  conventional  strength.  The  multilevel  pat- 
tern and  the  clinical  interview  will  usually  indicate  how  precarious  or 
brittle  these  surface  operations  are. 

In  summary  it  can  be  said  that  whatever  the  reason  bringing  the 
hypernormal  patient  to  the  clinic,  it  is  seen  by  him  as  an  annoying  ap- 
pendage separate  from  his  perception  of  his  own  personality.  This 
situation  makes  the  "88"  patient  a  particularly  tricky  prognostic  prob- 
lem. 

Standard  Psychiatric  (Kraepelinian)  Equivalents 
of  the  Hypernormal  Personality 

There  appears  to  be  no  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis  which  covers 
the  behavior  described  in  this  chapter.  Psychiatric  literature  has  tra- 
ditionally neglected  the  normal,  the  superior,  and  the  supernormal 
personality,  and  those  who  present  these  operations. 

Before  the  increasing  popularity  of  psychosomatic  concepts,  the 
"88"  personality  type  did  not  appear  in  the  psychiatric  consulting 
rooms.  The  early  psychiatric  theories  and  nosologies  were  clinical  in 
origin.  The  overtly  strong,  popular,  protective  personality  failed  to 
receive  conceptual  attention. 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic  (because  of  its  consultative  relation 
to  a  general  hospital)  has  evaluated  hundreds  of  patients  whose  overt 
and  conscious  behavior  is  hypernormal.  The  Foundation's  research  in 
psychosomatic  medicine  has  collected  multilevel  test  batteries  on  more 
than  one  thousand  of  these  cases. 

Analysis  of  these  protocols  has  led  us  to  view  strong,  affihative, 
supportive  security  operations  not  as  ideal  or  normal  ways  of  behaving 


320  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

but  as  machinery  for  warding  off  anxiety,  avoiding  disapproval,  and 
raising  self-esteem.  There  are  several  hundred  multilevel  patterns  of 
behavior  which  can  underly  a  hypernormal  fagade.  Some  of  these 
patients  might  be  given  psychiatric  or  psychosomatic  labels  (depend- 
ing on  their  specific  symptomology).  Some  of  them  are  psychotic 
individuals  (usually  paranoid)  who  desperately  cling  to  an  overt  ap- 
pearance of  adjustment.  Many  of  them  would  remain  undiagnosed 
according  to  current  psychiatric  nosology  and,  if  labeled  at  all,  would 
be  called  "normals." 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  Hypernormal  Personality 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  has  studied  several  hundred  sub- 
jects with  the  Level  I  diagnosis  of  responsible-hypernormal  personal- 
ity.  Here  is  a  summary  of  current  findings. 

1.  Forty-three  per  cent  of  all  patients  with  psychosomatic  symp- 
toms fall  in  the  NO  octant  at  Level  L  Psychosomatic  patients  there- 
fore use  these  hypernormal  operations  three  or  four  times  more  than 
chance  expectancy. 

2.  Fourteen  per  cent  of  nonpsychosomatic  psychiatric  patients  fall 
in  this  sector.  These  patients,  therefore,  do  not  use  these  operations 
more  than  chance  expectancy. 

3.  Hypernormal  operations  are  characterized  by  the  following 
MMPI  pattern:  high  scores  on  hypochondriasis  (Hs),  repressive 
blandness  (Hy),  denial  of  pathology  (K),  low  scores  on  nonconform- 
ity (F),  depression  (D),  schizoid  (Sc),  and  obsessive  tendencies  (Ft). 

4.  These  patients  are  not  well  motivated  for  psychotherapy.  They 
rank  as  the  lowest  group  in  average  number  of  therapy  sessions.  This 
indicates  that  they  refuse  treatment  or  quit  soon  after  beginning. 
Pure  or  stable  hypemormals  remain  in  treatment  on  the  average  of  six 
sessions.  Conflicted  hypernormals  remain  in  treatment  about  twice  as 
long  (average  equals  eleven  sessions).  Here  the  underlying  trends  (of 
weakness  or  bitterness)  make  them  more  likely  to  remain  in  therapy. 

5.  They  are  highly  identified  with  their  parents.  In  our  study  of 
conscious  identification  with  mother,  father,  and  spouse,  they  rank 
first:  and  on  another  study,  second  among  the  eight  diagnostic  groups. 

6.  Hypernormal  patients  (along  with  hysterics)  tend  to  misper- 
ceive  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  others  in  a  consistent  direction. 
They  attribute  too  much  friendliness  and  cooperativeness  to  others. 
They  also  tend  to  see  others  as  stronger  than  they  are  consensually 
judged  to  be.  This  indicates  that  responsible  personalities  consistently 
tend  to  see  others  as  like  themselves — falsely  perceiving  others  as  more 
loving  and  strong  than  they  are.  This  is  unquestionably  a  function 
of  their  attempt  to  maintain  a  bland,  conventional  atmosphere  which 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  RESPONSIBILITY  321 

fails  to  take  into  account  the  actual  amount  of  hostility- weakness  pres- 
ent in  others  as  well  as  themselves. 

7.  The  hypernormal  personality  type  appears  in  certain  cultural 
and  institutional  samples  much  more  frequently  than  in  others.  The 
percentage  of  responsible  individuals  in  various  samples  at  Level  I-M 
is  presented  in  Table  24. 

TABLE  24 

Percentage  of  RESPONSlBLE-HYPER^"ORM^L  Personalities   (Level  I-M) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

7o  of  Respoiisible-Hypernormal 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
College  Undergraduates 
University  Psychiatric  Clinic 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Fem.ilc) 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Ulcer  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patients 
University  Counseling  Center 
University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 
Stockade  Prisoners  (Male) 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Pauents 
OflScers  in  Military  Service 

Total  1903 

The  percentage  figures  expected  by  chance  for  these  groups  is  12.5. 
The  psychiatric  sample  contains  more  than  the  expected  percentage 
because  the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic  services  a  general  hospital  and 
four  additional  medical  centers.  The  number  of  patients  with  somatic 
and  psychosomatic  referrals  is  much  greater  than  that  seen  in  the 
standard  psychiatric  clinic.  The  clinic  policy  of  referring  certain 
naive,  conventional,  or  hypernormal  patients  to  group  therapy  for 
educational  reasons  accounts  for  the  fact  that  a  higher  percentage  of 
responsible  patients  are  seen  in  group  therapy. 

In  general  it  will  be  noted  that  the  noncHnic  samples  of  "normal" 
subjects  (e.g.,  army  officers)  contain  three  to  four  times  the  expected 
percentage  of  hypernormal  subjects. 

8.  The  percentage  of  responsible-hypernormal  individuals  in  var- 
ious samples  at  Level  II-C  is  presented  in  Table  25.  It  will  be  observed 
that  the  psychosomatic  samples  tend  to  have  three  times  the  expected 
number  of  hypernormal  patients.    The  ulcer  sample  and  the  neuro- 


537 

22 

415 

33 

133 

31 

121 

36 

23 

33 

29 

109 

19 

12 

54 

48 

38 

39 

31 

25 

28 

14 

39 

49 

322  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

dermatitis  samples,  whose  respective  tendencies  towards  aggression 
and  masochism  have  been  previously  noted,  are  exceptions  to  this 
generalization.  The  fact  that  a  fairly  high  percentage  of  psychotics 
claim  to  be  hypernormal  is  an  interesting  finding,  the  significance  of 
which  is  discussed  in  Chapter  23. 

TABLE  25 

Percentage  of  Responsible-Hypernormal  Personalities    (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Responsible-Hypernormal 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patients 
Ulcer  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

Total  781 

9.  Hypernormal  patients  see  their  fathers  as  exceedingly  strong- 
conventional  people.  The  father  is  consciously  idealized.  The  mother 
is  seen  as  extremely  loving,  tender,  and  agreeable.  A  most  conven- 
tional portrait  of  both  parents  is  produced.  They  also  see  their  marital 
partners  as  conventional,  friendly — but  slightly  more  docile  than  their 
conscious  picture  of  their  mothers.  The  conscious  descriptions  of  all 
three  family  members  are  located  on  the  conventional  side  of  the 
diagnostic  grid. 

10.  Of  all  the  "88"  patients  seen  in  the  psychiatric  clinic  over  a  one 
year  period,  74  per  cent  did  not  go  into  psychotherapy.  By  compari- 
son only  46  and  48  per  cent  of  distrustful  and  masochistic  patients  did 
not  go  into  therapy.  This  lends  empirical  support  to  the  statement 
that  hypernormal  subjects  are  not  initially  well  motivated  for  psycho- 
therapy. 


207 

IS 

46 

15 

101 

16 

38 

11 

31 

13 

56 

21 

70 

33 

41 

27 

42 

14 

49 

35 

100 

36 

20 

Adjustment  Through  Power: 
The  Autocratic  Personality 


Power,  success,  and  ambition  as  means  of  warding  off  anxiety  and  in- 
creasing self-esteem  comprise  the  theme  of  this  chapter.  We  shall  con- 
sider those  individuals  whose  overt  interpersonal  operations  stress 
compulsive  energy,  authority,  and  dominance  over  others.  This  is  the 
"11"  personality  type. 

Until  recently,  these  patients  were  not  often  seen  in  psychiatric 
consulting  rooms.  The  nature  of  their  security  operations  is  such  that 
they  were  not  seen  as  needing  psychiatric  help  and  would  hardly  con- 
sider asking  for  help. 

Adaptive  Forms  of  the  Power-Oriented  Personality 

Adjustment  through  power  can  be  an  adaptive  and  successful  way 
of  life.  Included  here  are  those  persons  who  express  strength,  force, 
energy,  and  leadership,  and  who  win  from  others  respect,  approbation, 
and  deference. 

The  generic  idea  of  hero  belongs  to  this  mode  of  adjustment.  So 
do  all  forms  of  ambition.  So  do  the  traits  of  energy,  planful  organiza- 
tion, and  righteous  authority.  Behavior  which  is  designed  to  excite 
admiration  or  to  provoke  submission  from  others  can  be  considered 
as  diagnostic  of  this  security  operation. 

There  are  many  ways  in  which  power  can  be  manifested.  Physical 
strength,  especially  in  the  case  of  the  male,  is  a  means  of  winning 
respect.  Intellectual  strength  is  another  common  power  operation. 
The  sage,  the  wise  man,  and  the  savant  are  all  roles  which  earn  respect. 
The  interpersonal  mechanism  of  teaching  is,  in  fact,  probably  the  most 
common  manifestation  of  power  motivation.  The  ordinary,  common- 
place frequency  of  the  teaching  behavior  makes  its  power  implications 
go  unnoticed.  It  seems  clear,  however,  that  whenever  one  person  be- 

323 


3H 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


gins  to  instruct,  inform,  or  explain  to  another,  he  is  conveying  the 
interpersonal  message,  "I  know  something  you  do  not  know,  I  am 
wise  and  better  informed  on  this  subject  than  you."  Intellectuals  are 
often  power-oriented  individuals  who  maintain  illusions  of  strength 
and  prestige  through  their  knowledge.  The  nonintellectual  who  can- 
not understand  why  teachers  seek  out  and  remain  in  positions  of  such 
low  pay  may  fail  to  recognize  the  rewards  and  securities  which  accrue 
to  the  pure  undiluted  power  expression  of  the  pedagogue. 

Teaching  is  thus  a  most  adaptive  and  constructive  manifestation  of 
the  autocratic  impulse. 

In  addition  to  physical  and  intellectual  strength,  there  are  several 
other  ways  in  which  power  can  be  gained  and  expressed.  Social 
status  is  perhaps  one  of  the  most  effective  means  of  exerting  authority. 
Prestige — either  bureaucratic  or  social — is  a  power  magnet  for  at- 
tracting respect  and  deference. 

Financial  strength  is  another  common  form  of  power  expression. 
Most  forms  of  conspicuous  consumption  are  diagnostic  of  the  attempt 
to  maintain  a  superior  (22)  or  powerful  (11)  fa9ade. 

In  summary  it  can  be  said  that  the  "11"  personality  is  characterized 
by  energetic,  organized  behavior,  by  the  attitude  of  knowledge,  com- 
petence, strength,  and  authority. 

Maladaptive  Forms  of  the  Poiver-Oriented  Personality 

The  extreme  forms  of  this  way  of  life  are  characterized  by  auto- 
cratic, domineering  behavior.  Compulsive  attempts  to  control  are 
diagnostic  of  this  maladjustment.  So  is  power-ridden,  overambitious 
behavior.  Pedantry  falls  into  this  category;  as  do  status-driven  at- 
tempts to  impress. 

The  person  who  tries  to  overorganize  his  life  and  the  lives  of  those 
around  him  is  utilizing  maladaptive  power  operations.  The  compulsive 
person  is  often  striving  to  increase  his  fa9ade  of  competence  and  ef- 
ficiency. His  exaggerated  attempts  to  be  planful,  precise,  and  correct 
are  diagnostic  of  the  "11"  maladjustment. 

The  key  factor  in  this  maladaptive  type  is  the  complete  avoidance 
of  weakness  and  uncertainty,  and  the  compulsive  endeavor  to  appear 
competent,  organized,  and  authoritative. 

The  autocratic  person  exhibits  his  power-oriented  machinery  of 
adjustment  rigidly  whether  it  is  appropriate  to  tne  situation  or  not. 
He  cannot  relax  his  compulsive,  energetic  operations.  In  social  or 
recreational  contexts  he  grimly  clings  to  his  mantle  of  efficiency  and 
competence  however  uncalled-for  it  may  be.  The  extremes  of  thi^ 
type  of  maladjustment  often  involve  hyperactivity  and  manic  be- 
havior. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  POWER  325 

In  the  clinic  the  autocratic  individual  is  thus  easily  diagnosed  by  his 
inappropriate  responses.  He  does  not  act  like  a  patient  coming  for 
help — but  as  a  strong  competent  person  seeking  to  inform  or  impress 
the  clinician. 

The  Purpose  of  "11"  Behavior 

Individuals  select  power-oriented  security  operations  because  they 
have  found  them  to  be  effective  in  warding  off  anxiety.  They  feel 
secure  when  they  are  exerting  control  over  people  and  things.  They 
apparently  dread  the  possibility  of  being  weak,  uninformed,  submis- 
sive. 

The  rewards  and  comforts  which  can  be  obtained  through  control 
and  power  are  numerous.  The  strong  person  feels  defended  and  pro- 
tected. He  wins  awe,  admiration,  and  obedience  from  others.  He 
gains  a  feeling  of  certitude  and  organization — which  serve  as  an  il- 
lusory buffer  against  the  mysteries  and  uncontrollable  possibilities  of 
existence. 

The  autocratic  individual  is,  we  assume,  made  most  anxious  when 
he  feels  uncertain,  confused,  or  passive.  He  attempts  to  maintain 
security  and  self-esteem  and  to  avoid  derogation  and  hurt  by  means  of 
his  power-oriented  operations. 

The  Effect  of  "11"  Behavior 

The  fagade  of  power  and  control  provokes  others  to  obedience, 
deference,  and  respect  from  others.  This  is  to  say,  AP  pulls  //. 

In  most  situations  the  person  who  manifests  wisdom  is  looked  to 
for  advice.  The  person  who  demonstrates  planful  control  and  compe- 
tence is  respected. 

This  principle  of  reciprocal  interpersonal  relations  is,  of  course,  a 
probability  statement.  It  can  be  altered  by  the  personality  of  the  other 
person.  Thus  a  managerial  person  interacting  with  another  who  uses 
the  same  interpersonal  reflexes  may  generate  a  power  struggle.  He 
may  receive  agreeable  cooperation  from  a  person  with  hysterical  op- 
erations. 

In  general  it  will  be  found  that  rigid  autocratic  individuals  seek  out 
docile  admiring  followers.  They  are  most  comfortable  when  they 
are  paired  with  those  who  symbiotically  match  their  interpersonal 
reflexes — who  flatter,  obey,  and  respect  them. 

Clinical  Manifestations  of  Managerial  Power 

It  has  been  pointed  out  that  prior  to  the  1930's  the  managerial  per- 
sonality was  not  a  frequent  visitor  to  the  psychiatric  clinic.  In  recent 
years,  however,  a  broader  definition  of  neurosis  (as  any  form  of  ex- 


326  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

treme  or  maladaptive  behavior)  has  developed.  In  addition,  the  con- 
cepts of  psychosomatic  medicine  have  stressed  the  point  that  certain 
physical  symptoms  can  be  manifestations  of  maladaptive  conflicts. 

For  these  reasons,  more  and  more  patients  whose  overt  fagade 
stresses  power  and  energy  are  being  referred  for  psychiatric  diagnosis. 
There  are  several  specific  clinical  characteristics  of  the  power-oriented 
personality. 

Psychosomatic  symptoms  are  a  most  common  complaint.  Ulcer 
patients  are  classically  seen  as  driving,  ambitious,  energetic  people. 
Certain  dermatitis  diagnostic  groups  utilize  strong  interpersonal  re- 
flexes. Overweight  women  tend  to  present  clinically  in  the  same  man- 
ner. Asthmatic  men  tend  to  stress  power  and  deny  weakness  in  their 
approach  to  a  psychiatric  clinic. 

Some  strong  managerial  individuals  come  to  the  clinic  because  of 
their  concern  about  other  family  members.  One  frequent  type  of 
referral  involves  the  competent,  industrious  woman  who  is  married  to 
a  weak,  delinquent,  or  rebellious  husband.  The  managerial  wife  comes 
partially  seeking  the  clinic's  support  in  getting  her  husband  into  treat- 
ment and  partially  because  of  her  own  underlying  passive  needs.  This 
type  of  strong  woman  inevitably  manifests  "preconscious"  masochism 
and  is  usually  involved  in  a  complicated  guilt-power  conflict  with  her 
husband. 

Many  cases  of  alcoholism  or  gambling  present  a  power-oriented 
facade  to  the  clinic.  These  patients  see  their  symptomatic  behavior  as 
isolated  from  their  character  structure  and  are  not  initially  well 
motivated  for  therapy.  The  prognosis  in  these  cases  depends  upon 
the  ability  to  tolerate  consideration  of  their  underlying  rebellious  or 
passive  feelings. 

Another  symptom  typical  of  the  "11"  personality  involves  an  iso- 
lated anxiety  attack.  The  patient  regularly  uses  compulsive,  energetic, 
self-confident  operations  to  handle  anxiety.  This  fagade  may  tem- 
porarily crack  (in  response  to  a  particularly  threatening  environmental 
circumstance).  The  patient  comes  to  the  clinic  because  he  is  scared 
by  the  possibility  of  a  recurrence.  (An  anxiety  attack  or  any  other 
sign  of  weakness  is,  of  course,  the  most  paralyzing  catastrophe  to  the 
person  who  utilizes  power  security  operations.)  By  the  time  the 
patient  comes  for  his  intake  interview,  his  routine  compulsive  reflexes 
may  be  working  smoothly  again.  He  mobilizes  against  the  threat  of 
anxiety  created  by  psychological  exploration  and  presents  a  fa9ade  of 
competent  strength.  These  patients  see  their  anxiety  attacks  as  iso- 
lated events,  not  integral  to  their  strong  character  structure.  The  lat- 
ter they  do  not  usually  want  to  change. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  POWER  327 

Some  managerial  personalities  (male)  come  to  the  clinic  with  symp- 
toms of  impotency.  The  fagade  of  strength  is  particularly  disturbed 
by  sexual  inadequacy.  Generally  these  patients  are  eager  to  have  the 
symptoms  (which  are  uncomfortable  signs  of  weakness)  removed  and 
are  not  pressing  to  explore  the  underlying  passivity  or  fear  which  the 
symptoms  represent. 

(Occasionally  some  "11"  types  come  to  the  clinic  because  of  dis- 
satisfaction with  their  interpersonal  relationships  with  others.  The 
competent  wife  puzzled  by  her  errant  husband  has  been  mentioned. 
The  compulsive,  righteous  husband  frustrated  by  a  rebellious  wife,  or 
by  resentful  children,  is  another  example.  Now  and  then  compulsive 
patients  come  under  pressure  from  their  employers  who  threaten  to 
fire  them  because  of  friction  generated  by  their  power  strivings.  A 
particularly  sad  variety  of  managerial  operations  is  afforded  by  the 
masculine,  driving  woman  who  finds  herself  lonely  and  neglected  by 
men  and  who  hopes  to  find  relief  from  her  vague  dissatisfaction  with 
self  without  relinquishing  her  compulsive  protections. 

There  is  one  exception  to  this  generalization.  Some  highly  intelli- 
gent, psychologically  sophisticated  individuals  come  to  the  clinic  seek- 
ing intensive  treatment  or  psychoanalysis.  These  patients  are  actu- 
ally hoping  to  change  their  character  structure.  They  may  have  some 
of  the  symptoms  mentioned  above  and  are  insightful  enough  to  want 
therapeutic  help.  These  patients  are  intellectually  (and  not  emotion- 
ally) motivated  for  psychotherapy.  They  will  exhibit  their  power  re- 
flexes but  have  enough  insight  to  ask  for  and  remain  in  treatment. 
Such  patients  are  usually  referred  to  psychoanalysts  or  assigned  to 
intensive  psychotherapy. 

Relatio?2ship  of  Power-Oriented  Personality 
to  Standard  Psychiatric  Diagnostic  Types 

Although  adjustment  (or  maladjustment)  through  power  has  not 
classically  been  the  focus  of  much  psychiatric  theory,  there  are  two 
diagnostic  types  which  have  some  of  these  interpersonal  factors  im- 
plicit in  their  definition. 

The  compulsive  personality  seems  to  involve  definite  power  mo- 
tives. The  compulsive  person  is  one  who  is  active,  prompt,  well-or- 
ganized, industrious,  pedantic,  planful,  and  often  righteously  compe- 
tent. The  person  who  exhibits  these  traits  is  clearly  trying  to  impress 
others  with  his  effectiveness.  (The  fact  that  he  is  generally  trying  to 
deal  with  his  own  inner  feelings  of  guilt  or  impotency  may  appear  in 
the  form  of  multilevel  conflicts  which  often  characterize  the  com- 
pulsive patients  seen  in  the  clinic.) 


-28  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

Successful,  well-adjusted  compulsives  are  generally  respected  by 
others  for  their  diligence  and  organization.  The  notion  of  efficiency 
(for  the  American  and  German  cultures,  at  least)  is  heavily  loaded 
M^ith  power  connotations.  In  Chapter  16  we  have  attempted  to  dis- 
tinguish between  the  obsessive  and  the  compulsive  modes  of  adjust- 
ment. It  was  suggested  there  that  these  two  behaviors  are  quite  dis- 
tinct in  terms  of  symptom  and  interpersonal  meaning  to  others. 

The  obsessive  person  usually  presents  as  guilty,  passive,  and  un- 
certain. The  compulsive  as  strong  and  right.  While  their  security  op- 
erations are  different,  they  can  sometimes  be  seen  in  the  same  per- 
sonality pattern,  usually  when  the  compulsive  defenses  are  breaking 
down. 

The  specific  power  elements  of  the  compulsive  state  have  not  been 
made  the  central  diagnostic  key — usually  being  subordinated  to 
symptomatic  factors.  Some  of  the  dominance-submission  aspects  of 
compulsivity  are  implicit  in  certain  psychoanalytic  writings.  Freud's 
first  and  most  authoritative  paper  on  the  compulsive  character  was 
published  in  1908  (2).  At  this  time  he  presented  his  conception  of 
the  three  anal  characteristics:  orderliness,  parsimony,  and  obstinacy. 
In  the  paper  "Character  and  Anal  Eroticism"  he  first  described  the 
first  great  power  struggle  of  life:  children's  "great  self-will  about 
paning  with  their  stools."  He  then  describes  the  parents  typical  at- 
tempt to  "break  his  (the  child's)  self-will  and  make  him  submissive." 
Fenichel  (1,  p.  280)  sees  the  anal  character  trait  of  orderliness  as  "the 
elaboration  of  obedience." 

Most  psychoanalytic  writers  tend  to  agree  as  to  the  power  elements 
of  the  compulsive  personality  but  draw  psychosexual  rather  than  inter- 
personal conclusions.  Compulsivity  is  thus  seen  as  a  reaction  forma- 
tion against  the  child's  stubborn,  managerial  wish  to  foil  the  parent  by 
soiling. 

Mullahy  (3,  p.  61)  has  presented  a  summary  of  the  resolution  of 
this  archaic  power  struggle  which  is  very  congenial  to  the  inter- 
personal theory.  He  points  out  the  strivings  for  "self-determination" 
associated  with  anal  activities  and  then  makes  the  additional  (and 
crucial  remark) :  "When  the  child  succeeds  in  making  a  virtue  out  of 
necessity,  he  is  said  to  identify  himself  with  the  requirements  of  his 
educators  and  is  proud  of  his  attainment.  Thus,  the  primary  injury  to 
his  narcissism  is  compensated,  and  the  original  feeling  of  self-satisfac- 
tion in  being  'good.'  " 

Compulsivity  (through  identification)  thus  provides  the  individual 
with  the  feehng  of  power  and  righteousness. 

This  relation  between  self-satisfaction  and  power  is  confirmed  by 
the  empirical  findings  of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research.  Managerial 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  POWER  329 

personalities  are  most  closely  identified  (consciously)  with  their  par- 
ents. They  are  closely  identified  with  their  ego  ideals.  They  are 
pleased  with  themselves. 

Obsessive  patients,  on  the  contrary,  are  the  most  self-disapproving 
and  are  least  identified  consciously  with  their  ego  ideals. 

A  clinical  description  of  a  personality  type  which  seems  similar  to 
the  managerial  personality  has  been  presented  by  Frank  et  al.  (4,  p. 
215)  They  entitle  this  behavior  pattern  the  doctor'' s  assistant,  which 
they  say,  ".  .  .  consists  of  a  patient's  tendency  in  the  group  to  de- 
fend authority,  to  please  the  doctor,  to  offer  advice  to  other  patients, 
to  hide  his  own  weaknesses,  and  generally  to  impress  everyone  with 
his  own  excellence.  This  behavior  springs  from  an  idealization  of 
authority  in  general  and  a  conviction  that  the  way  to  win  an  author- 
ity's good  will  is  to  demonstrate  one's  loyalty  and  excellence." 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  Managerial  Personality 

The  Kaiser  Foundation  research  has  studied  over  2,000  psychiatric 
clinic  patients  and  over  1,000  psychosomatic  and  normal  subjects. 
The  managerial  type  (at  Level  I)  comprises  the  largest  percentage  of 
cases  studied.  Over  600  patients  who  employ  these  security  operations 
have  been  diagnosed.  We  shall  now  consider  some  of  the  current 
research  findings  characteristic  of  this  personality  type. 

1.  Twenty-three  per  cent  of  patients  with  psychosomatic  symp- 
toms (i.e.,  ulcer  or  hypertensive)  fall  into  the  managerial  sector  at 
Level  L  Only  8  to  13  per  cent  of  nonpsychosomatic  patients  (i.e.,  pa- 
tients with  classic  neurotic  symptomatology)  are  given  the  diagnosis  of 
power-oriented  personality.  This  tends  to  confirm  the  suggestion  that 
managerial  patients  do  not  tend  to  come  to  the  clinic  or  enter  psycho- 
therapy with  overt  psychopathological  symptoms. 

2.  Patients  who  consciously  describe  themselves  as  managerial  have 
a  characteristic  MMPI  profile.  They  manifest  high  scores  on  the 
hyperactivity  scale  (Ma)  and  the  denial  of  symptoms  scale  (K),  and 
low  scores  on  depression  (D)  and  obsessive  tendencies  (Ft).  They  do 
not  stress  emotional  symptoms. 

3.  Managerial  patients  do  not  tend  to  enter  or  remain  in  psycho- 
therapy. They  are  seen  in  treatment  on  the  average  of  six  sessions. 
They  rank  lowest  (tied  with  hypernormal  and  narcissists)  on  number 
of  times  seen  in  the  psychiatric  clinic.  They  are,  therefore,  not  initially 
well  motivated  for  psychotherapy. 

4.  Managerial  patients  tend  to  be  closely  identified  (consciously) 
with  their  parents.  On  one  study  they  rank  first  in  closeness  of  identi- 
fication with  mother;  and  on  a  second  study  they  rank  second  on  this 
variable  (being  topped  only  by  hypernormals). 


330 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


5.  They  are  closely  identified  with  their  marital  partners.  They 
rank  second  in  this  variable.  Only  the  hypernormal  group  claims  a 
closer  connection  with  their  spouses. 

6.  Managerial  patients  (along  with  narcissists)  have  a  characteristic 
misperception  of  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  others.  They  attribute 
too  much  weakness  to  others  with  whom  they  interact.  They  seem  to 
look  down  on  others  and  fail  to  perceive  strength  in  others. 

7.  The  managerial  personality  tends  to  appear  in  certain  cultural 
and  institutional  settings  with  varying  frequencies.  Table  26  presents 
the  percentage  figures  for  Level  I-M.    The  percentage  expected  by 

TABLE  26 

Percentage  of  Managerial-Autocratic  Personalities   (Level  I-M) 

Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 


%  of  Mavagerml-Aiitocratic 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 

537 

15 

College  Undergraduates 

415 

45 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic 

15} 

13 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

121 

30 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

22 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

57 

25 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

71 

36 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 

109 

13 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

49 

8 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

18 

Ulcer  Patients 

43 

21 

Medical  Control  Patients 

37 

35 

University  Counsebng  Center 

93 

38 

University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 

39 

31 

Stockade  Prisoners  (Male) 

52 

19 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Paaents 

28 

14 

Officers  in  Military  Service 

39 

46 

Total 

1903 

chance  for  these  groups  is  12.5.  It  will  be  noted  that  all  groups  except 
the  two  psychotherapy  samples  contain  much  more  than  the  expected 
frequency  of  managerial  personalities.  The  norms  on  which  these  in- 
terpersonal diagnoses  are  based  were  taken  from  a  sample  of  807 
psychiatric  clinic  admissions.  The  results  listed  in  Table  26  suggest 
that  more  than  three  times  as  many  military  officers  manifest  power- 
oriented  operations  than  do  clinic  admission  patients.  Fifteen  per 
cent  of  admissions  to  the  Kaiser  Foundation  psychiatric  clinic  exert 
strong  compulsive  symptomatic  pressure  on  the  clinic.  Only  8  per 
cent  of  individual  therapy  patients  utilize  these  overt  operations,  which 
tends  to  confirm  the  statement  that  managerial  compulsive  patients 
come  to  the  clinic  for  diagnosis  (and  perhaps  symptomatic  relief)  but 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  POWER  331 

do  not  tend  to  enter  psychotherapy.  The  larger  percentage  ( 1 3  per 
cent)  of  managerial  patients  in  group  therapy  is  caused  by  the  chnic's 
policy  of  placing  certain  psychosomatic  patients  (e.g.,  ulcer  patients) 
in  group  treatment. 

8.  The  frequency  figures  for  the  occurrence  of  the  managerial  per- 
sonality at  Level  II-C  are  presented  in  Table  27.  Three  psychosomatic 
groups  (ulcer,  hypertensive,  and  obese)  claim  to  be  stronger  by  a  ratio 
of  over  4  to  1  than  normal  controls.  A  fairly  large  percentage  of 
psychotic  patients  attempt  to  maintain  the  conscious  illusion  of  execu- 
tive power.  Patients  who  end  up  in  individual  psychotherapy  are,  as 
noted  before,  docile  and  less  managerial  in  their  fagade  operations. 

TABLE  27 

Percentage  of   Managerial-Autocratic  Personalities   (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

%  of  Managerial-Autocratic 
Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample  N  Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions  207  12 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male)  46  17 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients  101  12 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients  38  8 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients  31  10 

Self -Inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients  56  13 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients  70  11 

Medical  Control  Patients  41  7 

Ulcer  Patients  42  33 

Hypertensive  Patients  49  33 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female)  100  32 

Total  781 

9.  Managerial  patients  tend  to  see  their  mothers  as  exceedingly 
strong,  independent  people.  They  also  describe  their  fathers  as  strong, 
but  not  as  powerful  as  their  mothers.  They  describe  their  marital  part- 
ners as  much  more  passive  and  agreeable  than  their  parents. 

10.  Sixty-eight  per  cent  of  all  managerial  patients  seen  in  the  psychi- 
atric clinic  (over  a  one-year  period)  did  not  go  into  treatment.  This 
indicates  that  this  personality  type  is  not  initially  well  motivated  for 
psychotherapy.  (By  contrast  46  per  cent  of  distrustful  patients  did 
not  go  into  therapy.) 

References 

1.  Fenichel,  O.   The  psychoanalytic  theory  of  neurosis.   Nev/  York:  Norton,  1945. 

2.  Freud,  S.   Character  and  anal  eroticism.   Collected  papers.   Vol.  2.   London:  Ho- 
garth Press,  1948. 

3.  Mullahy,  p.  Oedipus  myth  and  complex.  New  York:  Hermitage  Press,  1948. 

4.  Rosenthal,  D.,  J.  Frank,  and  C.  Nash.  The  self-righteous  moralist  in  early  meet- 
ings of  therapeutic  groups.  Psychiat.,  1954,  11,  No.  3,  215-23. 


21 

Adjustment  Through  Competition: 
The  Narcissistic  Personality 


In  this  chapter  we  shall  consider  a  way  of  life  which  is  based  on  com- 
petitive self-confident  narcissism.  This  is  the  "22"  mode  of  adjust- 
ment. This  personality  type  is  of  particular  interest  because  it  ap- 
pears very  rarely  in  the  psychiatric  clinic  and  has  been  given  scant 
theoretical  attention  in  proportion  to  the  frequency  of  its  occurrence. 

The  "22"  personality  expresses  at  Level  I  a  clear  love  and  approval 
of  himself.  He  acts  in  a  strong,  arrogant  manner.  He  communicates 
the  message  that  he  feels  superior  to  the  "other  one."  He  appears  in- 
dependent and  confident. 

In  its  adaptive  intensity  this  interpersonal  reflex  is  a  most  impres- 
sive social  maneuver.  In  its  maladaptive  extreme  it  becomes  a  smug, 
cold,  selfish,  exploitive  social  role.  In  this  case  the  adaptive  self-confi- 
dence and  independence  become  exaggerated  into  a  self-oriented  rejec- 
tion of  others.  The  individual  is  so  rigidly  tied  to  his  own  self-enhance- 
ment that  he  fails  to  sense  the  inappropriateness  of  his  behavior. 

Exhibitionism  and  proud  self-display  are  often  diagnostic  of  this 
personality  type.  This  competitive  attitude  may  show  itself  in  dress, 
carriage,  and  gesture,  or  in  the  purposive  meaning  of  verbalizations. 
The  kind  of  narcissistic  expression  varies  from  person  to  person.  Some 
narcissists  stress  their  intellectual  superiority.  Others  (more  typically 
women)  center  their  overt  narcissism  on  their  appearance,  dress,  and 
physical  beauty.  Conspicuous  consumption  is  generally  related  to 
this  security  operation  as  well  as  all  forms  of  snobbishness. 

The  Purpose  of  Competitive  Narcissism 

Narcissistic  displays  of  superiority  are  a  means  of  warding  off  anx- 
iety through  ascendance  and  self-enhancement.  These  individuals 
feel  most  secure  when  they  are  independent  of  other  people  and  feel 
they  are  triumphing  over  them. 

332 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COMPETITION  333 

These  individuals  depend  for  their  self-esteem  on  the  demonstra- 
tion of  weakness  in  others  and  competitive  strength  in  themselves. 
This  security  operation  is  close  to  the  managerial-autocratic.  The 
difference  lies  in  the  amount  of  positive  or  affiliative  affect  involved. 
The  executive  personality  vi^ants  loving  respect  and  obedience.  The 
narcissist  provokes  defeated  envy  and  inferiority  feelings.  The  nar- 
cissist puts  more  distance  between  himself  and  others — he  wants  to 
be  independent  of  and  superior  to  the  "other  one."  Dependence  is 
terrifying. 

Another  familiar  variety  of  the  competitive  mode  of  adjustment  is 
seen  in  the  case  of  the  status-driven  person.  These  individuals  are  un- 
usually sensitive  to  issues  of  superiority-inferiority.  They  invest  con- 
siderable energy  in  protecting  and  increasing  their  prestige.  This  con- 
cern with  status  is  also  typical  of  the  managerial  personality.  The 
latter,  however,  tends  to  provoke  others  to  yield  authority  to  him 
willingly  through  recognition  of  his  strength.  The  competitive  person 
is  generally  more  ruthless  and  exploitive  in  his  attempts  to  seize  and 
maintain  superiority  and  pulls  a  less  willing  submission  from  others. 
To  put  it  in  different  terms — the  managerial  person  trains  others  to 
identify  with  his  strength  and  gives  the  impression  that  his  power  will 
be  used  either  neutrally  or  to  help  the  weaker.  The  narcissistic  person 
tends  to  emphasize  his  superior  difference  from  the  "other"  and  gives 
the  impression  that  his  status  and  strength  will  be  used  to  shame  or  hu- 
miliate the  "other." 

Competitive  persons  are  apparently  made  most  anxious  by  the  threat 
of  weakness  or  dependence.  Weakness  is  generally  sensed  by  these 
individuals  to  be  a  dangerous  or  humiliating  position.  Often  the  experi- 
ences of  childhood  have  been  so  traumatic  as  to  lead  to  a  counteraction 
in  the  direction  of  strength.  In  other  cases  dependence  is  associated 
with  crucial  figures  with  whom  the  subject  desires  to  disidentify. 
Thus  the  counterphobic  man  equates  docility  with  passivity.  The 
competitive  woman  may  attempt  to  act  the  opposite  of  a  submissive 
parent  whose  passivity  is  consciously  perceived  as  a  negative  trait. 

The  specific  purpose  of  independent  arrogance  is  to  establish  a 
superior  invidious  relation  with  others.  These  subjects  apparently 
view  passivity,  cooperation,  trust,  or  tenderness  as  dangerous.  They 
seem  to  fear  the  loss  of  proud  individuality  which  is  attached  to  these 
other  operations. 

There  are,  of  course,  many  rewards  associated  with  self-confident 
narcissism.  Self-approval  can  be  a  pleasant  experience.  The  person 
who  bases  his  security  on  overt  independence  is  comforted  by  the 
satisfaction  in  flexing  his  muscles,  admiring  his  own  strength  or  beauty 
or  wisdom,  and  reveling  in  his  advantages  over  those  whom  he  per- 


334  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

ceives  as  inferior.  Adaptively  self-confident  individuals  receive  con- 
siderable admiration  and  social  approval. 

In  the  maladaptive  extreme,  the  narcissist  seems  driven  to  inflate 
himself  compulsively  at  the  expense  of  others.  This  brand  of  ab- 
normality leads  to  destructive  activities.  The  severe  narcissist  cannot 
tolerate  success  or  strength  in  others.  He  is  driven  to  compete,  to  ex- 
hibit, to  exploit.  He  is  consistently  rejecting  and  selfish.  His  com- 
pulsive and  frantic  attempts  to  boast  lead  to  a  most  unrewarding  circle 
of  activities.  As  the  narcissism  becomes  more  flagrant,  it  fails  to  win 
respect;  and  this  frustration  leads  to  increased  exhibitionistic  maneu- 
vers. 

The  maxim  of  this  form  of  maladjustment  is:  "How  can  1  establish 
superiority  over  this  person?  How  can  I  defeat  him?  How  can  I  use 
him  for  my  selfish  enhancement?" 

The  Effect  of  "22"  Behavior 

Competitive,  self-enhancing  behavior  pulls  envy,  distrust,  inferior- 
ity feelings,  and  respectful  admiration  from  others.  In  the  language 
of  the  interpersonal  system,  BC  provokes  GHIJ. 

The  adaptive  person  who  uses  this  security  operation  in  a  sensitive 
manner  wins  the  admiration  and  flattering  envy  of  others.  They  look 
up  to  him  and  pay  him  the  tribute  of  a  grudging,  envious  approval.  It 
may  be  helpful  to  contrast  the  interpersonal  world  created  by  the 
competitive  person  with  the  response  which  the  executive,  dominating 
person  provokes.  The  latter  is  more  conventional  and  responsible  in 
his  use  of  power.  He  tends  to  train  others  to  obedience  or  loving 
respect.  The  competitive  person  strives  to  impress  others  that  he  has 
what  they  want.  There  is  more  disaffiliative  motive  in  his  approach 
and  he  generally  receives  therefore  a  passively  hostile,  negative  sub- 
mission. 

These  generalizations  are,  of  course,  probability  statements.  The 
response  of  the  "other  one"  is  determined  partly  by  his  own  inter- 
personal reflexes.  Thus  a  rigidly  docile,  agreeable  person  may  mani- 
fest the  most  friendly  responses  to  a  narcissist's  approach.  An  inflex- 
ibly competitive  person  will  react  to  another  narcissist  not  with  hum- 
ble defeated  envy  but  with  an  increase  in  his  own  independent  re- 
flexes. A  fierce  exhibitionistic  competition  between  the  two  often  re- 
sults. Thus  the  principle  of  reciprocal  relations  (which  in  this  case 
reads  BC  pulls  GHIJ)  will  be  found  to  work  in  most  cases  but  does  not 
hold  where  inappropriate  narcissism  characterizes  the  subject  or  other 
rigid  reflexes  characterize  the  other. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COMPETITION  335 

Clinical  Manifestations  of  Co?npetitive  Narcissism 

Narcissistic  patients  rarely  come  to  a  psychiatric  clinic  for  diag- 
nosis or  therapy.  In  one  study  of  537  routine  admissions  to  a  psychi- 
atric clinic  only  6  per  cent  were  diagnosed  competitive  or  narcissistic 
at  the  level  of  symptomatic  presentation.  There  are  fewer  narcissists 
in  clinic  samples  than  any  other  diagnostic  type. 

The  psychiatric  clinic  is  thus  not  the  natural  habitat  of  the  com- 
petitive, independent  person.  The  reason  for  this  finding  seems  clear. 
The  emphasis  on  proud  self-enhancement  is  quite  incongruous  with 
seeking  psychiatric  help.  The  very  essence  of  this  mode  of  adjustment 
is  that  "22's"  ask  help  from  no  one,  need  no  assistance,  and  are  getting 
along  quite  well  on  their  own  steam. 

The  "22"  patients  who  do  show  up  in  the  clinic  generally  come  for 
one  of  three  reasons:  (1)  psychosomatic  symptoms,  (2)  current  in- 
juries to  their  narcissism,  (3)  the  desire  to  display  their  personalities 
or  to  talk  about  themselves. 

Patients  who  are  referred  to  the  clinic  for  ulcer  or  asthmatic 
symptoms  often  present  independent,  narcissistic  fa9ades.  The  diag- 
nostic location  for  the  average  ulcer  patient  (at  Level  II-C)  is  in  the 
BC  (narcissistic-competitive)  octant.  Many  of  these  patients  tend  to 
stress  proud,  hardboiled  self-sufficiency.  The  same -is  true  of  asthmatic 
patients. 

The  second  group  of  competitive  patients  seen  in  the  clinic  are 
those  whose  self-regard  has  received  a  recent  defeat.  They  often  re- 
port the  most  colorful  and  fearful  symptomology.  They  often  list 
dozens  of  symptoms  and  may  recount  their  eccentricities  and  life  his- 
tories in  great  detail.  The  superficial  impression  of  depression  or  de- 
pendence is  deceptive.  Psychological  testing  or  perceptive  interview- 
ing will  reveal  that  the  patients  are  not  as  anxious  or  depressed  as  they 
appear.  What  becomes  evident  is  a  narcissistic  concern  with  their  own 
reactions,  their  own  sensitivities.  The  precipitating  cause  for  their 
entrance  to  the  clinic  is  usually  a  shift  in  their  life  situation,  which 
causes  frustration  or  a  blow  to  their  pride.  The  birth  of  a  child  may 
cause  the  narcissistic  woman  to  become  upset  over  the  new  demands 
of  responsibility,  nurturance,  and  the  loss  of  attention.  Narcissistic 
people  in  general  react  negatively  to  parenthood  and  intense  conflicts 
may  appear  in  this  connection.  One  way  in  which  this  conflict  can 
be  handled  is  for  the  subject  to  incorporate  the  child  into  the  circle 
of  his  or  her  own  narcissism  and  thus  share  attention  with  the  child. 

Occupational  changes  which  lower  public  esteem  or  create  de- 
pendency or  require  a  tender  approach  may  produce  tensions  in  com- 


J 36  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

petitive  men.  Phallic,  exhibitionistic  men  are  often  forced  to  retire  to 
more  sedentary,  conventional  occupations  with  accompanying  pain 
and  tension.  This  phenomenon  was  quite  common  after  the  last  war 
when  aviators,  combat  soldiers,  etc.,  were  faced  with  the  loss  of  the 
grarificatiops  of  their  positions. 

The  histrionic  character  of  this  personality  type  often  leads  such 
individuals  into  activities  which  involve  public  display — modeling, 
acting,  and  other  forms  of  social  exhibition.  Any  shift  in  their  life 
situation  which  involves  the  relinquishment  of  these  rewards  can 
make  tension  and  symptoms  and  lead  to  psychiatric  referral. 

Many  competitive  men  are  most  comfortable  when  they  are  hold- 
ing independent  positions — running  their  businesses,  etc.  Failures  in 
those  activities,  which  create  a  feeling  of  weakness  or  require  them  to 
take  subordinate  posts,  can  lead  to  increased  anxiety  and  possibly 
physical  symptoms. 

Another  very  common  reason  which  brings  the  narcissistic  persons 
into  the  psychiatric  clinic  is  their  intense  interest  in,  concern  for,  and 
love  of  themselves.  Many  people  perceive  therapy  as  a  unique  oppor- 
tunity to  talk  about  themselves,  to  spin  theories  about  themselves, 
and  to  engage  the  interest  and  attention  of  a  respected  person  (the 
therapist)  in  the  subject  that  is  dearest  to  the  narcissist's  heart. 

These  three  factors — physical  symptoms,  narcissistic  injury,  and 
self-fascination — seem  to  account  for  the  motivation  of  those  few  com- 
petitive characters  who  come  to  the  clinic.  They  lead  to  the  paradoxi- 
cal situation  of  patients  who  are  not  really  depressed  or  dependent 
applying  for  psychiatric  help. 

Narcissists  do  not  provide  difficult  problems  for  diagnosis  if  the 
intake  worker  focuses  on  the  purposive  meaning  of  their  communica- 
tions and  is  not  diverted  by  dramatic  (but  not  deeply  felt)  symptomol- 
ogy.  These  patients  are  trying  to  impress  the  "other  one."  They  may 
do  this  by  muscle-flexing,  boasting,  seductive  and  colorful  case 
histories,  flirtatious  maneuvers,  or  outright  competition  with  the  clini- 
cian. 

There  are  certain  psychometric  signs  characteristic  of  this  personal- 
ity type.  On  the  MA4PI,  the  anxiety  and  passivity  scales  (D  and  Ft) 
are  low.  The  imperturbability  scale  (Ma)  is  high.  Physical  symptoms 
may  push  the  Hs  scale  up.  The  conventionality-isolation  scales  are 
neither  markedly  high  nor  low.  Thus  F  and  Sc  are  not  as  high  as  in 
the  case  of  the  psychopath  and  schizoid.  The  conventionality  scales 
K  and  Hy  are  not  as  pronounced  as  in  the  case  of  the  psychosomatic 
and  hysteric. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COMPETITION  337 

Standard  Psychiatric  Definition  of  the  Narcissistic  Maladjustment 

In  most  of  the  preceding  diagnostic  chapters  it  has  been  possible  to 
relate  the  interpersonal  type  of  maladjustment  to  a  standard  psychi- 
atric category.  Distrust  defines  the  schizoid;  docile  dependency  de- 
fines the  phobic,  etc.  This  relationship  between  interpersonal  and 
psychiatric  diagnosis  does  not  hold  in  the  case  of  the  autocratic  per- 
son, who  has  received  relatively  little  attention  from  clinical  theorists. 
These  dominating,  power-oriented  persons  do  not  tend  to  come  for 
help,  and  have  thus  been  neglected  in  the  psychiatric  literature. 

The  same  situation  holds  for  the  "22"  personality.  There  is  objec- 
tive evidence  indicating  that  this  personality  type  does  not  often  come 
to  the  cHnic.  There  is,  therefore,  not  a  commonly  agreed  or  Krae- 
pelinian-type  term  for  categorizing  these  persons. 

This  mode  of  maladjustment  has,  however,  not  been  completely 
neglected  by  psychiatric  writers.  The  psychoanalytic  theory  tends  to 
focus  not  on  the  symptomatic  factors  (which  are  stressed  by  the  pre- 
analytic  psychiatrists)  but  stresses  the  multilevel  aspects  of  character 
structure.  This  much  more  sophisticated  approach  considers  character 
traits  as  means  of  warding  off  anxiety  or  instincts.  The  psychoanalysts 
have  always  recognized  that  self-love  and  independent  narcissism  form 
a  common  and  eifective  way  of  warding  off  or  counteracting  under- 
lying feelings  of  weakness. 

The  term  counterphobic  is  often  employed  to  describe  the  exhi- 
bitionistic  personality  who  compulsively  attempts  to  demonstrate  his 
superiority. 

Fromm  has  defined  the  exploitive  character  as  one  who  attempts 
to  better  himself  at  the  expense  of  others. 

Horney  sees  narcissism  as  one  of  the  basic  neurotic  "trends."  The 
narcissistic  person,  as  defined  by  Horney,  inflates  himself  and  ag- 
grandizes himself  at  the  expense  of  others. 

Jerome  Frank  and  his  colleagues  have  described  three  behavior  pat- 
terns seen  in  psychotherapy  groups  which  are  very  close  to  three  of 
the  interpersonal  types  presented  in  this  book.  The  help-rejecting 
complainer  and  the  doctor's  assistant  have  been  referred  to  in  Chapter 
15  and  Chapter  21,  respectively. 

A  third  interpersonal  type  isolated  by  Rosenthal,  Frank,  and  Nash 
(1,  pp.  217-18)  is  called  the  self-righteous  moralist.  This  mode  of  be- 
havior is  quite  similar  to  that  being  described  in  this  chapter.  These 
authors  describe  this  type  as  follows: 

The  most  outstanding  characteristic  of  the  self-righteous  moralist,  as  ex- 
emplified by  these  patients,  is  the  need  to  be  right  or  to  show  up  the  other 
fellow  as  wrong,  particularly  when  some  moral  issue  is  involved  which  im- 
pinges on  his  own  system  of  values,  .  .  . 


338  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

In  the  very  first  group  meeting,  the  self-righteous  moralist  tends  to  present 
himself  as  one  who  is  calm,  controlled,  and  self-contained,  indicating  his  su- 
periority by  a  show  of  poise.  He  usually  manages  to  become  the  focus  of  the 
discussion  by  his  intensity,  by  dramatizing  whatever  he  has  to  say,  and  by 
laboring  his  position  indefinitely,  refusing  to  concede  any  point,  to  admit  any 
error,  or  to  make  any  modification  of  his  original  formulation.  .  .  . 

When  symptoms,  problems,  and  personal  history  are  discussed,  he  talks  of 
these  in  such  a  way  as  to  enhance  his  own  status:  for  example,  he  says  that  he 
has  survived  worse  distress  than  others;  that  he  has  carried  on  in  his  duties 
despite  his  illness;  that  others  are  sicker  than  he  is;  and  that  others  can  profit 
from  learning  how  he  has  handled  his  problems.  .  .  . 

Schafer  (2)  has  given  more  attention  to  the  narcissistic  personality 
than  any  other  chnically  oriented  writer.  As  diagnostic  cues  he  stresses 
"striking  egocentricity,"  a  tendency  to  avoid  anxiety-arousing  situa- 
tions (i.e.,  they  do  not  like  to  exhibit  behavior  HI  on  the  diagnostic 
circle).  Schafer  also  mentions  exhibitionism  and  overdemonstrative- 
ness,  which  he  believes  to  be  a  cover-up  of  "basic  coldness  and  dis- 
tance." In  general  it  appears  that  the  character  disorder  defined  by 
Schafer  is  close  to  the  narcissistic  type  of  maladjustment  described  in 
this  chapter. 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  Narcissistic  Personality 

In  the  preceding  discussion  of  the  narcissistic  personality  we  have 
leaned  upon  and  referred  obliquely  to  research  findings  of  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  project.  Some  of  these  results  will  now  be  summarized. 


TABLE  28 

Percentage  of  Competitive-Narcissistic 

Personalities 

(Level  I-M) 

Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

% 

of  Comp 

etttive-Narcissistic 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission 

537 

6 

College  Undergraduates 

415 

9 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic 

133 

10 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

121 

10 

Overdy  Neurotic  Dermatids  Patients 

31 

0 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

57 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

71 

Group  Psychotherapy  Pauents 

109 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

49 

10 

Hypenensive  Patients 

49 

Ulcer  Patients 

43 

Medical  Control  Pauents 

37 

University  Counseling  Center  (Male) 

93 

University  Graduate  Smdents  (Male) 

39 

21 

Stockade  Prisoners  (Male) 

52 

13 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients 
Officers  in  Military  Service 

28 

11 

39 

0 

Total 

1903 

ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  COMPETITION  339 

1.  Patients  who  manifest  competitive  operations  at  Level  I  (MMPI) 
and  in  the  rated  interpersonal  reflexes  (sociometrics)  do  not  have 
psychosomatic  symptoms  (except  for  the  ulcer  group). 

2.  Ulcer  patients  are  the  psychosomatic  group  who  stress  com- 
petitive independence  in  their  Level  II  self-descriptions. 

3.  Competitive  patients  have  MMPI  profiles  with  low  scores  on 
depression  (D)  and  obsessive  rumination  (Pt)  and  relatively  higher 
scores  on  manic  imperturbability  (Ma). 

4.  These  patients  do  not  tend  to  come  to  the  psychiatric  clinic. 
This  diagnostic  group  is  the  least  likely  to  accept  a  psychiatric  referral. 

5.  This  personality  type  is  found  in  other  cultural  samples  more 
frequently  than  in  the  psychiatric  clinic.  As  indicated  in  Table  28 
there  are  ten  samples  which  contain  more  narcissists  at  Level  I-M 
than  the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic  admission  group.  University  gradu- 
ate students  contain  the  most  competitive  personalities,  followed  by 
stockade  prisoners  and  university  psychiatric  clinic  patients.^ 

6.  The  Level  II-C  self-diagnoses  of  several  samples  are  listed  in 
Table  29.  Comparison  of  Tables  28  and  29  is  made  difficult  by  the 
fact  that  the  two  samples  which  contained  the  greatest  number  of 
narcissists  at  Level  I-M  (graduate  students  and  prisoners)  were  not 
included  in  the  Level  II-C  study.  At  the  level  of  conscious  self- 
description,  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis  patients  claim  the  most  com- 
petitive self-confidence  and  the  group-therapy  patients  (who  em- 
phasize schizoid  distrust)  and  self-inflicted  dermatitis  patients  (who 
stress  conventionality)  claim  the  least  narcissism. 

7.  Narcissists  are  not  especially  motivated  for  psychotherapy.  One 
sample  of  these  patients  came  on  the  average  for  six  therapeutic  inter- 
views. This  ties  them  for  last  place  among  diagnostic  groups  in  terms 
of  length  of  treatment.  Female  narcissists,  incidentally,  seem  to  stay 
in  therapy  longer  than  male  narcissists. 

8.  Competitive  patients  tend  to  be  consciously  disidentified  with 
their  parents.  They  are  more  identified  than  schizoids  and  psycho- 
paths but  clearly  less  identified  than  the  managerial,  psychosomatics, 
hysterics,  and  phobics. 

9.  The  same  findings  hold  for  conscious  marital  identifications. 
10.  Narcissistic  patients  tend  to  describe  their  parents  as  being 

relatively  sadistic. 

*  The  fact  that  the  military  officer  sample  contains  no  narcissistic  subjects  would 
seem  to  be  a  contradiction  to  the  previous  statements  which  claimed  that  exhibitionis- 
tic  characters  are  often  located  in  military  pursuits.  The  military  officer  group  in- 
cluded here  was  tested  under  assessment  circumstances  which  probably  influenced 
their  test-taking  attitude  and  their  resulting  symptomatic  scores.  These  officers  were 
assessed  in  a  nonclinical,  quasi-military  situation  where  there  would  be  little  pressure 
to  stress  narcissistic,  unconventional  feelings  and  some  motivation  to  emphasize  re- 
sponsible executive  traits,  which  they  did. 


%  of  Co?npetitive-Narctssisttc 

N 

Personalities 

207 

14 

46 

11 

101 

7 

38 

11 

31 

17 

56 

9 

70 

11 

41 

10 

42 

14 

49 

12 

100 

12 

340  INTEEIPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

TABLE  29 

Percentage  of  Competitive-Narcissistic  Personalities    (Level  II-C) 
Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

Instimtional  or  Sy?nptomatic  Sample 
Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 
Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 
Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 
Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 
Self-inflicted  Dermautis  Patients 
Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 
Medical  Control  Patients 
Ulcer  Patients 
Hypertensive  Patients 
Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

Total  781 

11.  They  describe  their  marital  partners  as  agreeable,  admiring 
people.  This  suggests  that  they  tend  to  marry  people  whom  they  see 
as  weak,  docile,  and  who  will  pay  them  respectful  tribute. 

12.  On  the  Naboisek  study  of  interpersonal  misperception  the  com- 
petitive-exploitive  group  (along  with  the  managerials)  reveals  marked 
misperceptions  of  weakness  in  others.  They  attribute  too  much  passiv- 
ity and  too  much  hostility  to  weak  people.  This  suggests  that  an  un- 
usually intense  contemptuous  superior  attitude  may  exist  in  relation  to 
weakness  in  others. 

References 

1.  Rosenthal,  D.,  J.  Frank,  and  E.  Nash.  The  self-righteous  moralist  in  early  meet- 
ings of  therapeutic  groups.  Psychiat.,  1954.    17,  No.  3,  215-23. 

2.  Schafer,  Roy.    The  clinical  application  of  psychological  tests:  Diagnostic  sum- 
maries and  case  studies.   New  York:  International  Universities  Press,  1948. 


22 

Adjustment  Through  Aggression: 
The  Sadistic  Personality 


The  next  sector  of  the  diagnostic  continuum  is  the  area  of  critical  hos- 
tile aggression.  We  shall  consider  in  this  chapter  those  human  beings 
who  manifest  in  their  overt  operations  cold  sternness,  punitiveness,  or 
sadism.  This  is  the  "33"  personality. 

This  way  of  life  is  traditionally  one  of  the  most  fascinating  and 
disturbing.  We  are  dealing  here  with  the  fearful  and  destructive  as- 
pects of  human  behavior.  We  shall  attempt  to  understand  why  some 
individuals  select  negative,  hostile  expressions  as  their  means  of  ad- 
justment. 

Philosophers  and  psychologists  have  for  centuries  recognized  that 
many  human  beings  are  compulsively  committed  to  aggression.  Many 
theories  have  been  advanced  to  explain  why  some  persons  deUght  in 
combat,  feel  comfortable  only  when  engaged  in  a  threatening  attack, 
experience  no  qualms  at  punishing  their  fellows,  and,  indeed,  feel 
weakened  and  threatened  by  the  prospect  of  collaborative  or  tender  or 
docile  impulses. 

An  important  point  must  be  introduced  at  this  early  stage  of  the 
discussion.  We  are  referring  in  this  chapter  not  just  to  actions  of 
criminal  aggression,  destructive  violence,  or  socially  disapproved 
sadism.  We  include  all  those  behaviors  which  inspire  fear  in  others, 
which  threaten  others  by  physical,  moral,  or  verbal  means. 

Many  antisocial  individuals  utilize  this  hostile  mode  of  adjustment. 
But  the  great  majority  of  punitive  sadistic  characters  are  to  be  found 
in  the  ranks  of  the  socially  approved.  Those  persons  who  consistently 
maintain  a  punishing  attitude  towards  others,  or  a  disciplinary  atti- 
tude, or  a  sarcastic  attitude,  or  a  guilt-provoking  attitude  fall  in  this 
diagnostic  category.  Stern  toughness  is  frequently  admired  and  en- 
dorsed as  a  positive  social  adjustment. 

34' 


342  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

Those  individuals  who  become  repetitiously  engaged  in  physical 
violence  would,  of  course,  be  given  the  interpersonal  diagnosis  of  ag- 
gressive personality.  But  a  large  percentage  of  the  cases  falling  in  this 
diagnostic  category  do  not  go  around  punching  others — they  com- 
municate their  critical,  hostUe  messages  in  more  subtle,  but  equally 
effective  means.  We  think  here,  for  example,  of  the  stern  unforgiving 
father,  the  bad-tempered  wife,  the  moralistic  guilt-provoking  mother, 
the  sharp-tongued  mocking  husband,  the  grim-faced  punitive  official, 
the  truculent  fiery-natured  colleague,  the  disciplinarian.  We  include 
all  those  law-abiding,  often  pious  and  self-righteous,  individuals  who 
maintain  a  role  of  potential  insult,  derogation,  or  punishment. 

As  we  shall  see  in  the  subsequent  sections,  this  mode  of  adjustment 
is  far  from  being  limited  to  the  delinquent  margins  of  society.  It  ap- 
pears with  frightening  regularity  in  the  ruling  groups  of  most  societies 
— present  and  past — expressed  in  the  philosophy  of  repressive  legis- 
lation and  bellicose  foreign  policies. 

In  the  next  few  pages  we  shall  propose  some  speculations  about  the 
purpose,  the  effect,  the  survival  advantages  and  disadvantages  of  ag- 
gressive security  operations. 

The  Purpose  of  "33"  Behavior 

Those  individuals  who  are  overtly  hostile  and  punitive  have  se- 
lected these  behaviors  because  they  sense  them  to  be  the  most  effective 
in  minimizing  anxiety.  These  interpersonal  reflexes  communicate  a 
message  of  hardboiled  toughness:  "I  am  a  dangerous,  fearful  person." 
The  persons  who  rely  on  these  operations  for  their  emotional  security 
are  least  anxious  when  they  are  flexing  their  muscles  or  expressing 
stern  coldness.  They  are  made  most  anxious  in  a  situation  which  pulls 
for  tender,  agreeable,  or  docile  feelings. 

These  individuals  have  developed  their  involuntary  interpersonal 
reflexes  because  they  have  learned  consciously  or  unwittingly  that  this 
is,  for  them,  the  safest  mode  of  adjustment.  When  they  are  acting 
tough  or  stern,  they  feel  protected.  When  they  act  unaggressive,  they 
feel  unprotected  and  painfully  uncomfortable. 

Sadistic,  tough  human  beings  apparently  find  security  and  pleasure 
in  acting  hardboiled;  their  self-respect  seems  to  stem  from  the  provoca- 
tion of  fear  in  others.  Hurtful,  mocking,  destroying,  threatening  ac- 
tions endow  the  actor  with  a  fearful  power.  The  threat  of  a  temper 
outburst  or  a  savage  attack  is  a  forceful  weapon  for  coercing  and 
managing  others.  Even  the  less  violent  aspects  of  this  interpersonal 
operation — critical,  disciplinary  behavior — carry  an  authoritative  so- 
cial weight. 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  AGGRESSION  343 

Hostile,  critical  conduct  is  generally  viewed  as  negative  and  ethi- 
cally lamentable.  Despite  this  moral  censure  this  deportment  is  ac- 
companied by  feehngs  of  righteousness.  The  most  bitter  delinquent, 
as  well  as  the  most  punitive  disciplinarian,  often  justifies  his  transac- 
tions by  pious  reasons.  Sadists  thus  do  not  always  feel  the  pain  of  guilt 
or  the  whip  of  social  disapproval.  In  fact  they  often  fit  themselves  into 
contexts  where  harshness  and  coercion  are  admired  or  accepted.  The 
aggressive  criminal  gains  respect  in  his  own  society.  The  martinet 
wins  esteem  within  his  own  sphere  of  activities.  The  common  genus 
of  household  sadist  usually  operates  in  reciprocal  relationship  to  maso- 
chistic marital  partners,  who  respond  submissively. 

Another  very  important  purpose  of  punitive  or  critical  behavior  is 
the  provocation  of  guilt.  The  generic  function  of  the  hostile  way  of 
life  is  to  destroy,  to  humiliate,  to  cow  the  "other  one."  This  can  be 
done  violently.  It  can  also  be  done  indirectly.  The  cold,  stern,  dis- 
approving attitude  has  the  aim  of  making  the  "other  one"  feel  either 
inferior  or  unworthy.  The  sense  of  righteousness  and  austere  punitive- 
ness  is  a  most  common  and  forceful  attitude.  Moral  coercion  is  a  most 
effective  and  self-satisfying  form  of  sadism  since  it  allows  the  release 
of  destructive,  hostile  feelings  along  with  the  comforting  support  of 
self-approval. 

The  Effect  of  "3  3"  Behavior 

We  have  seen  that  stern,  hostile  interpersonal  reflexes  serve  several 
important  purposes.  They  can  reduce  anxiety  and  the  feeling  of  de- 
fenselessness.  They  express  a  feeling  of  armed  protection,  righteous 
irritation,  and  physical  or  moral  superiority  and  force. 

This  powerful  social  maneuver  has  quite  a  consistent  effect  on  other 
people  in  general  and  certain  rebellious  or  masochistic  people  in  par- 
ticular. We  shall  consider  first  the  general  case. 

Sadistic-critical  behavior  pulls  resentment,  distrust,  fear,  and  guilt 
from  "others."   In  systematic  language  DE  provokes  FGH. 

The  punitive,  hostile  role  is  a  most  effective  interpersonal  instru- 
ment. In  the  basic  sense  everyone  fears  destruction.  Physical  danger 
is,  of  course,  the  most  crude  and  direct  threat  to  any  living  organism. 
Social  danger  is,  for  the  human  being,  a  most  fearful  menace.  This  is 
expressed  generically  as  disapproval  or  derogation.  Almost  everyone 
dreads  and  resents  criticism  and  hostile  laughter  from  others.^ 

Hostile  coerciveness  thus  exerts  a  tremendous  interpersonal  lever- 
age. It  gains  a  fearful  respect  or  a  resentful  submission.  Moral  deroga- 

*  An  exception  to  this  generalization  is  furnished  by  the  overt  masochistic  charac- 
ter, cf.  the  discussion  of  the  hostihty-provoking  buffoon  m  Chapter  16. 


344  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

tion  provokes  guilt,  and  thus  possesses  a  ruling  force  which  can  equal 
or  surpass  the  threat  of  physical  violence. 

In  the  moderate  form,  the  critical  role  yields  advantages  to  both 
the  actor  and  the  "other."  No  institution  exists  which  does  not  depend 
to  a  certain  extent  upon  social  disapproval  as  a  cementing  and  centrif- 
ugal agent.  The  stern,  judicial,  punitive  person  thus  becomes  a  hu- 
man symbol  of  the  rules  and  sanctions  which  exist  either  explicitly  or 
implicitly.  The  critic  or  disciplinarian  serves  a  healthy  function  in  the 
economy  of  the  group  and  gains  respect  and  security  for  himself. 
Flexibility  and  adaptive  moderation  again  become  the  criteria  which 
differentiate  the  adjustive  from  the  maladjustive. 

In  many  famihes  one  of  the  parents  generally  pre-empts  this  role, 
and  thus  gains  the  fearful  respect  and  gives  the  reassurances  of  limits 
to  the  others.  The  well-adjusted  aggressive-punitive  person  does  not 
rely  on  these  interpersonal  reflexes  rigidly.  He  can  shift  to  other  be- 
haviors when  they  are  appropriate  and  when  the  critical  functioning 
is  not  called  for. 

In  the  extreme  form,  the  sadistic  role  becomes  the  nucleus  of  com- 
plex neurotic  phenomena.  The  maladjusted  aggressive ;  person  is  the 
one  who  manifests  this  operation  inflexibly  and  to  an  intense  degree. 
He  operates  as  though  anxiety  is  associated  with  the  relaxing  of  tough- 
ness and  this  anxiety  he  cannot  tolerate. 

Extreme  or  consistent  sadism  has  a  most  electrifying  effect  on  the 
"other  one."  Most  people  are  made  uncomfortable  and  ill  at  ease  in 
the  presence  of  an  explosive  or  condemnatory  or  sarcastic  person. 
They  tend  to  fear  him  and  to  avoid  him  when  possible. 

Most  individuals  can  tolerate,  and  even  appreciate,  the  function  of 
an  adjusted  critic.  They  cannot  tolerate  potential  or  actual  hostile 
coercion  in  others.  This  is  to  say  that  when  extreme  D  behavior  pulls 
adaptive  withdrawal  and  bitter  disaffiliation  from  "others,"  the  inter- 
action terminates. 

There  are  two  general  occasions  when  this  pattern  does  not  hold: 
in  the  case  of  the  reciprocal  sado-masochistic  relationship  and  in  the 
crime-punishment  partnership.  There  exists  a  large  number  of  indi- 
viduals who  are  most  comfortable  (although  not  necessarily  happy) 
when  they  are  tied  to  a  hostile  partner.  A  most  common  variety  of  this 
is  found  in  the  masochists.  An  intense  symbiotic  relationship  exists  be- 
tween those  who  are  least  anxious  when  hurting  or  derogating  and 
those  who  are  least  anxious  when  receiving  these  negative  actions.  In 
this  case  DE  pulls  intense  maladaptive  and  rigid  GH. 

Elaborate  multilevel  patterns  exist  in  the  sado-masochistic  relation- 
ship. A  maladaptive  rigid  sadistic  fagade  usually  covers  underlying 
feelings  of  fear  and  weakness.  These  are  neutrahzed  by  the  comfort- 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  AGGRESSION  345 

ing  protection  of  hardboiled  operations.  The  overt  hostility  in  turn 
breeds  guilt  and  a  fear  of  retaliation  which  leads  to  an  intensification 
of  the  original  reflex.  Similarly  the  overt  masochist  inevitably  pos- 
sesses "preconscious"  sadistic  identifications.  The  masochist  provokes 
hostility  from  the  "other"  which  is  generally  followed  by  the  provoca- 
tion of  guilt  in  the  aggressor.  The  anxiety  associated  with  this  hostile 
or  righteous  maneuver  usually  results  in  a  resumption  of  the  maso- 
chistic operations. 

The  multilevel  interactions  of  couples  who  are  involved  in  sado- 
masochistic locks  is  one  of  the  most  interesting  and  complex  human 
relationships.  The  delicate  interaction  between  the  two  forms  of 
sadism — physical  and  moral — are  nicely  illustrated  in  these  not 
atypical  cases. 

A  second  familiar  symbiotic  relationship  exists  in  the  intense  recip- 
rocal partnerships  between  rebels  and  punitive  authorities,  between 
criminals  and  the  agents  of  punishment.  It  is  well  known  that  irra- 
tionally unconventional  and  antisocial  individuals  pull  hostility  from 
others  (cf.  Chapter  15).  The  alienated  schizophrenic,  by  means  of  his 
purposive  eccentricity,  provokes  society  to  incarcerate  him.  The  re- 
bellious student  trains  his  teachers  to  discipline  him.  The  professional 
radical  eagerly  searches  his  atmosphere  for  evidence  of  repressive 
cruelty  (e.g.,  racial  discrimination)  and  often  succeeds,  not  in  helping 
his  cause,  but  in  gaining  the  condemnation  of  others.  Brilliant  crea- 
tivity often  reaches  its  peak  in  reaction  to  hostile,  unsympathetic,  re- 
strictive regulations.  The  other  (punitive)  side  of  this  crime-punish- 
ment partnership  works  with  equal  purposiveness.  Hostile  punitive 
people  seek  out  rebellious  and  distrustful  others  and  integrate  durable 
relationships  with  them.  Policemen  look  for  crime.  The  disapproving 
moral  sadist  looks  for  sinners.  The  bully  feels  most  comfortable  in 
receiving  the  resentful  reactions  of  those  he  coerces. 

Remarkably  intense  and  lasting  relationships  develop  between  anti- 
social rebels  and  the  punitive  figures  whose  anger  they  attempt  to  pro- 
voke. The  recidivist  criminal  is  least  anxious  when  he  is  deaUng  with 
the  comforting  consistency  of  prison  custody.  The  punitive  person 
is  most  comfortable  when  he  has  targets  for  his  hostility.  The  severely 
maladjusted  sadist  thus  gravitates  towards  bitter,  guilty,  and  fearful 
"others." 

Clinical  Manifestations  of  the  "33"  Personality 

The  sadistic  type  may  be  known  by  his  symptoms.  These  pa- 
tients do  not  manifest  the  depressive  characteristics  of  schizoids,  ob- 
sessives,  and  phobics.  They  are  not  loaded  with  worries.  They  do 
not  complain  of  physical  symptoms. 


346  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

They  come  to  the  clinic  usually  under  the  pressure  of  unsatis- 
factory interpersonal  relationships.  Alarital  problems  are  very  com- 
mon. Discord  and  friction  in  their  jobs  frequently  are  mentioned. 
Often  they  are  in  trouble  caused  by  their  hostility.  In  these  respects 
they  appear  much  like  the  rebellious  schizoid  patients.  Both  diag- 
nostic groups  emphasize  negative,  angry  interpersonal  reflexes.  The 
schizoids  are  passively  hostile,  while  the  sadists  are  actively  hostile. 
The  schizoids  are  mad  and  sad;  the  sadists  are  mad  and  not  sad.  They 
are  less  concerned  with  their  problems.  As  they  describe  their  life 
events,  a  note  of  contempt  and  disgust  with  others  often  develops. 
It  becomes  clear  that  they  are  giving  others  in  their  life  a  bad  time, 
that  they  are  looking  down  contemptuously  upon  others. 

These  patients  often  express  unconventional  ideas  and  admit  to 
unconventional  feelings.  They  may,  in  fact,  make  a  point  of  avoid- 
ing conventional  feelings  and  ideas,  and  when  they  do  employ  them 
they  are  often  used  to  derogate  others,  A  sadistic  wife  may,  for  ex- 
ample, admit  to  aggressiveness  in  herself  and  then  criticize  her  hus- 
band for  not  being  easygoing,  A  punitive  man  may  admit  his  own 
sexual  adventures  with  a  certain  hardboiled,  sophisticated  justification 
and  wax  indignant  at  the  misconduct  of  others. 

In  their  demeanor  during  diagnostic  interviews  these  patients  gen- 
erally manifest  aggressiveness  coupled  with  some  other  interpersonal 
role.  This  is  due  to  the  fact  that  pure,  unconflicted  aggressive  charac- 
ters rarely  come  to  a  psychiatric  clinic.  Invariably  the  aggressive  pa- 
tient presents  a  conflicted  fagade.  His  blunt,  tough  security  opera- 
tions have  led  to  trouble,  or  else  he  would  not  be  visiting  the  clinic. 
A4any  aggressive  patients  have  intense  covert  feehngs  of  weakness  or 
guilt.  These  may  be  apparent  in  the  clinical  interview.  The  brutal 
husband  may  express  verbal  guilt  for  beating  his  wife  or  children. 
The  aggressive  woman  may  verbalize  pious  conventional  feelings  in 
the  effort  to  prove  how  contemptible  her  husband  has  become.  The 
guilt  in  the  first  case  and  the  bland  conventionality  in  the  latter  will 
be  seen  to  be  superficial  and  verbal.  These  patients  may  sound  guilty 
but  they  are  not  depressed.  They  may  sound  cooperative  and  agree- 
able but  their  contemptuous  attitudes  will  be  picked  up  by  the  Level 
I  symptomatic  tests  (MMPI)  or  by  the  alert  interviewer.  These  feel- 
ings often  appear  in  the  form  of  sarcastic  or  depreciatory  references  to 
psychiatry  or  psychotherapy. 

It  has  been  pointed  out  that  "33"  characters  come  to  the  clinic 
complaining  of  interpersonal  problems  rather  than  anxiety  symptoms. 
There  are  certain  specific  familial  situations  which  are  typical  of  this 
personality  type.  The  blunt,  active,  righteous,  angry  wife  of  the 
delinquent  husband  is  one  such  case.    These  patients  often  calmly 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  AGGRESSION  347 

describe  a  long  history  of  marital  turmoil  in  which  a  weak,  immature 
spouse  repeatedly  offends  the  punitive  wife  with  chronic  alcoholism, 
gambling,  unemployment,  etc.  The  wife  often  supports  the  family 
and  rules  the  spouse  with  a  guilt-provoking  disciplinary  coldness  to 
which  the  husband  reacts  with  alternating  guilt  and  rebeUion.  These 
partnerships  often  are  of  long  standing.  The  punitive  member  comes 
to  the  psychiatric  clinic  in  the  wake  of  a  current  episode  of  inter- 
rupted rebellion  on  the  part  of  the  spouse.  The  motive  in  coming  may 
be  to  seek  help  in  dealing  with  the  husband,  rather  than  help  in 
changing  her  own  behavior.  The  feeling  of  righteous  indignation 
communicated  becomes  diagnostic.  Inevitably  testing  reveals  that 
masochistic  trends  underly  these  stern,  punitive  overt  operations. 
"Preconscious"  guilt  and  self -punishment  picked  up  by  fantasy  tests 
often  indicate  that  the  patient  is  close  to  recognition  of  the  underlying 
feelings  of  weakness,  and  these  may  provide  the  push  which  causes 
the  patient  to  come  to  and  stay  in  therapy.  Another  typical  com- 
plaint of  the  aggressive  character  involves  disgust  or  concern  over 
symptoms  in  children.  Delinquency,  bed-wetting,  and  phobias  often 
characterize  the  offspring  of  these  patients. 

A  third  reason  for  coming  to  the  clinic  concerns  authority  prob- 
lems. The  aggressive  person  often  finds  himself  in  a  jam  and  comes 
for  help  under  the  pressure  of  disciplinary  actions.  These  frictions 
are  usually  due  to  overharshness  with  subordinates,  quarrels  with 
equals,  or  insult  to  a  superior.  These  patients  do  not  manifest  real 
guilt  or  unhappiness  about  these  interpersonal  conflicts,  and  they  make 
it  clear  that  the  fault  lies  in  the  "other  one." 

There  are  psychometric  signs  diagnostic  of  the  stern  or  sadistic 
personality.  Their  MMPI  profiles  emphasize  peaks  on  the  hyper- 
mania,  psychopathic  deviate,  and  F  scales.  The  Sc  score  is  usually 
higher  than  Ft.  The  depression  score  is  not  pronounced.  The  Mf 
(femininity  score)  is  usually  low  for  male  patients  and  varies  for 
female  patients  depending  on  the  amount  of  underlying  masochism  or 
passivity. 

Interpersonal  Definition  of  the  Psychopathic  Maladjustment 

Evidence  has  been  presented  (cf.  Chapter  12)  that  certain  inter- 
personal maladjustive  types  were  related  to  psychometric  diagnostic 
categories. 

Hostile,  sadistic  security  operations  are  characteristic  of  the  psycho- 
pathic personality.  The  essence  of  the  psychopathic  state  is  active 
aggression.  These  patients  avoid  anxiety  and  maintain  security  by 
avoiding  dependent  or  tender  feelings  and  by  integrating  critical, 
punitive  relations  with  others. 


348 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 


The  classic  generalization  that  psychopaths  cannot  love  fits  the 
looric  of  the  circular  diagnostic  continuum  since  the  DE  octant  which 
defines  the  psychopath  is  exactly  opposite  the  affiliative  sector  of  the 
circle. 

Again  it  must  be  stressed  that  we  are  employing  a  definition  of  the 
kind  and  degree  of  abnormality  which  is  based  on  personal,  and  not 
cultural,  values.  The  cultural  definition  of  the  psychopathic  maladjust- 
ment stresses  the  inability  to  conform  to  social  norms.  This  is  a  poor 
definition  because  schizoid  characters  (as  defined  by  the  interpersonal 
system)  seem  to  get  into  trouble  as  frequently  as  psychopaths.  We 
have  already  stressed  the  point  that  many  sadistic  individuals  are 
quite  acceptant  of  punitive  and  repressive  ethical  values.  The  psycho- 
pathic personality  in  the  interpersonal  system  is  defined  by  the  afore- 
mentioned typical  security  operations  and  not  by  delinquency.  As 
a  matter  of  fact,  sadistic  people  are  perhaps  more  often  unusually 
identified  with  law  and  moral  codes  which  they  ruthlessly  employ  to 
humiliate  others.  The  more  a  person  goes  out  of  his  way  to  claim  an 
ethical  superiority  and  to  attribute  immorality  to  others,  the  greater 
the  probability  that  he  manifests  psychopathic,  morally  sadistic 
operations. 

The  distrustful  schizoid  patient  is  acutely  aware  of  moral  hypocrisy 
in  others.  Some  psychopaths  often  show  a  radar-like  sensitivity  to 
rebelliousness  and  guilt  or  weakness  in  the  "other  one." 

Research  Findings  Characteristic  of  the  Sadistic  Personality 

Some  of  the  current  research  findings  which  concern  the  aggressive 
personality  can  now  be  considered. 

L  Patients  who  manifest  stern  aggressiveness  in  their  overt  opera- 
tions do  not  have  psychosomatic  symptoms. 

2.  Psychosomatic  patients  do  not  utilize  these  interpersonal  opera- 
tions at  Levels  I  and  IL 

3.  Aggression  at  Levels  I  and  II  is  related  to  high  MMPI  scores  on 
nonconformity  (F),  schizoid  distrust  (Sc),  and  disidentification  with 
affiliative  values  (Pd). 

4.  If  sadistic  patients  enter  psychotherapy,  they  tend  to  remain 
in  treatment  for  long  periods.  They  stay  in  therapy  as  long  as  any 
other  diagnostic  type.  Severely  conflicted  psychopaths  (i.e.,  large 
discrepancies  between  Level  I  and  II)  are,  however,  poorly  moti- 
vated for  treatment,  remaining  on  the  average  for  only  two  sessions. 
This  indicates  that  the  psychopath  like  the  phobic  presents  a  tricky 
prognostic  gamble.  They  either  avoid  therapy  entirely  or  they  enter 
and  remain  for  extended  periods.  Multilevel  conflicts  can  lead  both 
of  these  groups  to  avoid  intensive  treatment.   Schizoid  and  obsessive 


ADJUSTMENT  THROUGH  AGGRESSION  349 

patients  on  the  contrary  are  more  likely  to  stick  in  treatment  regard- 
less of  the  degree  and  kind  of  multilevel  conflict. 

5.  The  psychopaths,  like  the  schizoids,  are  the  most  disidentified 
(consciously)  with  their  parents. 

6.  They  tend  as  a  group  to  be  consciously  disidentified  with  their 
marital  partners. 

7.  They  (along  with  the  schizoid  group)  tend  to  misperceive 
the  interpersonal  behavior  of  others.  They  inaccurately  attribute  too 
much  hostility  to  others. 

8.  Patients  who  manifest  aggressiveness  in  their  Level  I-M  sympto- 
matic behavior  tend  to  appear  in  certain  cultural  samples  much  more 
frequently  than  others.  The  percentage  of  sadistic  persons  in  various 
samples  is  presented  in  Table  30. 


TABLE  30 

Percentage  of  Aggressive-Sadistic  Personalities    (Level  I-M) 

Found  in  Several 

Cultural  Samples 

% 

of  Aggressive-Sadistic 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission 

537 

11 

College   Undergraduates 

415 

3 

University  Psychiatric  Clinic 

133 

11 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

121 

4 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

10 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

57 

2 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

71 

3 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 

109 

7 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

49 

6 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

2 

Ulcer  Patients 

43 

8 

Medical  Control  Patients 

37 

0 

University  Counseling  Center 

93 

2 

University  Graduate  Students  (Male) 

39 

0 

Stockade  Prisoners   (Male) 

52 

15 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients 

28 

11 

Officers  in  Military  Service 

39 

0 

Total 

1903 

As  might  be  expected,  the  stockade  prison  group  contains  the 
largest  percentage  of  psychopathic  individuals.  The  psychiatric 
clinic  samples  include  about  the  number  expected  by  chance.  All  the 
other  samples  have  a  negligible  number  of  this  personality  type. 

It  is  of  interest  to  note  the  discrepancy  between  psychopathic  per- 
sonalities reporting  to  the  clinic  and  those  going  into  group  or  indi- 
vidual psychotherapy.  Only  half  the  expected  percentage  of  sadistic 
individuals  go  into  treatment.  This  indicates  that  either  those  pa- 
tients tend  to  avoid  going  into  therapy  or  the  clinic  refrains  from  re- 


350  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY 

ferring  them  to  therapy.  They  come  to  the  clinic  in  the  expected 
frequency  but  their  reasons  for  coming  (which  often  involves  blam- 
ing others)  do  not  lead  them  to  go  into  therapy.  Once  they  do  enter 
treatment,  they  tend  to  stay  a  relatively  long  time.  This  means  that 
sadistic  patients  are  poorly  motivated  but  have  long  prognosis  for 
treatment. 

9.  The  percentage  of  sadistic  personalities  at  Level  II-C  is  pre- 
sented in  Table  3L  Ulcer  patients  claim  the  most  aggressiveness. 
Normal  controls  and  psychiatric  clinic  admissions  attribute  more 
sadism  to  themselves  than  expected  by  chance.  The  hypertensive  and 
obese  samples  (who  stress  hypernormal  strength)  have  considerably 
fewer  self-diagnoses  in  the  aggressive-sadistic  octant. 


TABLE  31 

Percentage  of  Aggressive-Sadistic  Personalities    (Level  II-C) 

Found  in  Several  Cultural  Samples 

% 

oj  Aggressive-Sadistic 

Institutional  or  Symptomatic  Sample 

N 

Personalities 

Psychiatric  Clinic  Admissions 

207 

18 

Hospitalized  Psychotic  Patients  (Male) 

46 

11 

Group  Psychotherapy  Patients 

101 

11 

Individual  Psychotherapy  Patients 

38 

8 

Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients 

31 

10 

Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients 

56 

14 

Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients 

70 

11 

Medical  Control  Patients 

41 

17 

Ulcer  Patients 

42 

24 

Hypertensive  Patients 

49 

4 

Middle  Class  Obese  Patients  (Female) 

100 

4 

Total 

781 

10.  Sadistic  patients  describe  their  parents  as  weaker  and  more 
neurotic  than  any  other  diagnostic  group.  They  clearly  look  down 
on  their  parents — seeing  them  as  beaten,  impotent,  timid,  unsuc- 
cessful, and  unloving  people. 

1 1 .  Sadistic  patients  describe  their  spouses  as  rebellious  and  resent- 
ful people.  This  indicates  that  their  marital  relations  are  loaded  with 
punitive  affect.  They  are  stern  and  disapproving  in  relation  to  resent- 
ful, passively  resistant  spouses. 


V 

Some  Applications  of  the  Interpersonal  System 


Introduction 


The  aim  of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  has  been  to  develop  a 
system  of  personality  which  is  functionally  useful  in  the  psychiatric 
clinic.  The  norms  have  been  based  on  clinic  samples.  The  empirical 
investigations  have  attempted  to  build  up  probability  statistics  which 
allow  us  to  predict  what  the  patient  will  do  in  the  clinic  setting. 

The  system  has,  for  the  first  six  years  of  research,  been  deliberately 
restricted  for  the  most  part  to  that  narrow  range  of  interpersonal  be- 
havior which  is  relevant  at  the  time  of  intake  evaluation  and  plan- 
ning for  psychotherapy.  The  major  appUcation  of  the  system  is  to 
problems  of  diagnosis  and  prognosis  faced  in  the  psychiatric  clinic. 

While  the  main  focus  has  been  on  cUnical  diagnosis,  there  have 
been  some  side  explorations  to  determine  the  efficacy  of  the  inter- 
personal system  in  predicting  behavior  outside  of  the  psychiatric 
clinic. 

In  the  next  four  chapters  we  shall  report  on  some  applications  of 
the  interpersonal  system  in  these  nonclinical  situations. 

Chapter  23  will  report  on  the  use  of  these  diagnostic  methods  in 
a  psychiatric  hospital.  The  interpersonal  pressures  faced  by  the  staff 
of  an  inpatient  service  are  clearly  different  from  the  outpatient  fa- 
cility. Our  experience  in  using  the  interpersonal  methodology  in 
this  situation  is  very  limited.  The  results  of  our  pilot  study  are  strik- 
ingly different  from  the  outpatient  studies  and  are  partially  con- 
firmed, moreover,  by  our  measurements  on  outpatients  who  have  had 
psychotic  breaks  and  required  hospitalization.  Some  suggestions  and 
impHcations  concerning  the  apphcation  of  interpersonal  diagnosis  in 
the  psychiatric  hospital  will  be  tentatively  advanced. 

Chapter  24  takes  us  to  a  different  environmental  setting — the  phy- 
sician's office.  We  shall  study  the  application  of  the  system  to  psycho- 
somatic problems  faced  by  the  internist  and  the  dermatologist.  Four 
samples  of  patients  manifesting  symptoms  which  are  sometimes  be- 
lieved to  be  psychosomatic  have  been  studied  by  the  Kaiser  Founda- 
tion research.  The  diagnostic  system  suggests  that  there  are  some 
personality  correlates  of  psychosomatic  conditions.  The  results  of 
these  studies  and  their  clinical  implications  for  the  physician  will  be 
reviewed. 

352 


INTRODUCTION 


353 


Chapter  25  describes  some  interpersonal  dynamic  factors  observed 
in  administrative,  discussion,  and  management  groups.  The  inter- 
personal measurement  methods  are  easily  converted  into  sociometric 
instruments.  Patterns  of  reciprocal  interpersonal  relations  and  mis- 
perceptions  of  self  and  others  are  measured  by  very  straightforward 
techniques.  They  provide  a  direct  method  for  diagnosing  an  industrial 
management  group  and  outlining  the  network  of  dynamic  activities 
which  occur  in  the  group  situation. 

In  Chapter  26  this  survey  of  the  application  of  the  diagnostic  sys- 
tem concludes  with  a  consideration  of  interpersonal  dynamics  as  they 
occur  in  group  therapy.  Methods  for  predicting  and  measuring  group 
resistance  and  group  personality  will  be  described.  This  chapter  also 
discusses  the  complex  issue  of  multilevel  interaction  patterns  as  they 
evolve  in  group  psychotherapy. 


23 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of 
Hospitalized  Psychotics 


The  emotional  atmosphere  in  any  psychiatric  hospital  is  inevitably 
different  from  that  of  the  outpatient  clinic.  Implicit  interpersonal 
forces  are  at  play  which  affect  the  patient's  behavior.  One  task  of  a 
diagnostic  system  employed  in  the  hospital  setting  is  to  measure  the 
patient's  reactions  to  the  social  pressures  of  the  hospital  environment. 
The  standard  questions  to  be  answered  by  a  multilevel  measurement 
apparatus  are:  What  overt  social  role  is  the  patient  attempting  to 
maintain  (i.e.,  what  are  his  interpersonal  security  operations)?  How 
does  he  diagnose  himself?  What  are  his  underlying  feelings? 

The  interpersonal  diagnostic  system  has  been  used  in  some  limited, 
exploratory  studies  in  a  hospital  setting.  Some  of  the  results  will  be 
reported  in  this  chapter.  These  findings  are  preliminary  and  sug- 
gestive. They  do  contain  several  implications  about  the  use  of  per- 
sonality tests  in  the  hospital  and  about  the  nature  of  the  psychotic  state. 

Factors  Unique  to  the  Psychiatric  Hospital 

In  assessing  the  interpersonal  behavior  of  hospitalized  patients  it  is 
clear  that  certain  factors  peculiar  to  institutional  commitment  are 
involved.  The  patients  are  certainly  involved  in  a  different  relation- 
ship with  the  therapist  than  are  patients  in  the  outpatient  clinic.  The 
latter  come  mainly  on  their  own  volition.  The  clinic  does  not  play 
such  a  vital  24-hour-a-day  role  in  their  lives.  The  outpatient  is  much 
more  free  to  sever  his  relationship  with  the  clinic. 

The  hospitalized  patient  is  inextricably  caught  in  a  web  of  inter- 
personal assumptions  which  affect  his  behavior  and  his  conception 
of  self.  He  is  legally  committed;  he  has  been  rejected  by  society,  and 
often  by  his  family.  He  is  not  as  free  to  govern  his  actions.  He  is  de- 
pendent on  the  institution  for  sustenance,  both  material  and  emotional. 

354 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS 


355 


The  interpretation  of  test  results  must  take  into  account  the  emo- 
tional context  of  the  hospital  scene.  We  can  never  be  sure  how  much 
the  patient's  behavior  is  directed  towards  the  therapist-as-therapist  and 
how  much  it  is  determined  by  his  attitudes  and  interpersonal  purposes 
towards  the  custodial  institution  and  the  rejecting  outer  world  to 
which  the  hospital  is  related. 

There  is  another  factor  which  limits  the  application  of  the  inter- 
personal system  to  hospital  diagnosis.  The  system  was  developed  to 
meet  the  needs  of  patients  in  a  clinic.  It  is  geared  to  patients  who  are 
of  average  intelligence  and  who  are  able  to  manage  their  affairs  by 
themselves.  Individuals  who  are  severely  psychotic  (i.e.,  out  of  touch 
with  reality,  which  usually  means  wildly  rebellious  against  conven- 
tional standards)  may  not  be  able  to  meet  the  intellectual  demands  of 
our  check  lists  and  questionnaires. 

For  this  reason  we  are  very  cautious  in  recommending  the  inter- 
personal system  for  general  use  in  the  psychiatric  hospital.  For  many 
patients  it  seems  to  work  with  adequate  success — that  is,  it  success- 
fully answers  the  question:  what  are  the  patient's  interpersonal  ac- 
tions, beUefs,  and  underlying  feelings? 

The  Psychotic  Samples 

Three  samples  of  psychotic  patients  were  studied  by  the  inter- 
personal diagnostic  system. 

The  State  Hospital  Sample  ^  comprises  100  patients  tested  at  Level 
I-M  and  6  patients  who  were  referred  for  group  psychotherapy  and 
were  administered  the  multilevel  interpersonal  test  battery  before  be- 
ginning treatment.  The  100  patients  were  a  random  sample  of  patients 
who  received  the  MMPI  during  diagnostic  work-ups. 

The  criteria  for  selecting  the  six  other  patients  were  as  follows: 
Seven  patients  were  assigned  to  the  therapy  group.  Six  of  them  took 
the  tests  and  received  interpersonal  diagnoses.  The  seventh  patient 
was  too  disturbed  to  respond  to  the  testing  situation.  All  group  mem- 
bers came  from  the  same  unit,  a  convalescent  cottage.  Five  had  re- 
ceived diagnoses  of  schizophrenia;  one  was  diagnosed  as  a  depressive 
psychotic.  None  were  currently  receiving  any  somatic  treatment. 
The  cottage  is  a  semi-open  ward  and  does  not  contain  acutely  dis- 
turbed patients.  While  the  rate  of  discharge  of  patients  in  this  unit  is 
relatively  high,  patients  selected  for  this  therapy  group  were  not  ex- 
pected to  be  leaving  the  hospital  within  the  next  four  months. 

*  The  sample  of  six  hospitalized  patients  was  collected  by  Richard  V.  Wolton  of 
the  Stockton  State  Hospital,  Stockton,  California.  Gratitude  is  expressed  to  Mr.  Wol- 
ton for  his  cooperation  in  administering  the  tests  and  for  writing  the  clinical  sum- 
maries included  in  this  chapter. 


356        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

A  second  sample  of  hospitalized  patients  has  been  studied  by  the 
interpersonal  diagnostic  system.  These  comprise  patients  who  were 
evaluated  in  an  outpatient  clinic,  diagnosed  either  as  psychotic  or 
anxiety-panic  types  and  then  hospitalized.  This  group  of  patients  is 
called  the  Clinic  Psychotic  Sample. 

There  are  22  subjects  in  this  "panic-psychotic"  sample,  4  men  and 
18  women.  Only  13  of  these  patients  completed  the  tests  at  all  three 
levels  so  that  the  N's  vary  from  level  to  level. 

The  Private  Hospital  Sample  includes  20  patients  who  were  in 
psychotherapy  at  the  Pinel  Foundation  Hospital,  Seattle,  Washing- 
ton.^ There  are  obvious  cultural  and  clinical  factors  which  might  dif- 
ferentiate private  hospital  patients  from  those  seen  in  a  state  hospital 
or  a  health-plan  chnic,  but  all  three  samples  share  the  common  ex- 
perience of  having  been  institutionalized  because  of  this  emotional 
symptom. 

Level  I  Behavior  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples 

Level  I-M  scores  are  available  for  the  state  hospital  and  clinic 
psychotic  sample.  Level  I-S  ratings  of  each  patient  by  professional  ob- 
servers (pooled  ratings  of  doctors,  nurses,  and  therapist)  are  available 
for  the  private  hospital  sample.  Table  32  presents  the  number  of  pa- 
tients falling  in  each  diagnostic  category  at  Level  L 

TABLE  32 
Level  I  Diagnoses  Assigned  to  148  Patients  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples 


Sample 

N 

Level  I  Diagnosis 
12      3      4      5      6      7      8 

Strong 

Loving 

1678 

HostUe 
Bitter 

2345 

State  Hospital  Sample  . 

Clinic  Sample 

Private  Hospital  Sample  * 

Total  Sample 

106 

22 
20 

148 

22     17     15      6      6     10      6     24 
2      2      3      4      3      3       14 
12      3      0      5      7      2      0 

25     21     21     10     14    20      9    28 

60 
10 
10 

80 

46 
12 
10 

68 

*  The  figures  presented  for  the  private  hospital  sample  are  based  on  Level  I-S 
(raters'  sociometric  checks).  The  other  two  samples  were  studied  at  Level  I-M— the 
symptomatic  level. 

No  clear-cut  trend  is  evident  in  these  results.  The  findings  sug- 
gest that  hospitalized  patients  present  a  mixed  picture  at  the  sympto- 
matic or  overt  interpersonal  level.   When  the  diagnostic  continuum 

^  Gratitude  is  expressed  to  the  administration  of  the  Pinel  Foundation  Hospital  and 
to  Dr.  Arthur  Kobler  of  the  Pinel  Staff  for  permission  to  use  the  results  obtained  in 
their  diagnostic  studies. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS 


357 


is  summarized  in  terms  of  positive  conventional  types  (1678)  as  com- 
pared with  hostile  alienated  types  (2345),  there  are  slightly  more 
psychotic  patients  in  the  former  category.  In  light  of  the  nature  of 
this  sample  this  becomes  a  most  interesting  result.  A4ore  than  half  of 
these  patients  who  have  been  rejected  by  society  and  institutionalized 
for  emotional  disturbance  present  themselves  as  responsible,  hyper- 
normal,  or  conforming  people.  The  implication  is  that  many  psy- 
chotics  strive  to  maintain  a  Level  I  fagade  of  conventionality  and  con- 
formity. 

The  findings  listed  in  Table  32  tend  to  duplicate  the  census  of  pa- 
tients in  the  Kaiser  Foundation  outpatient  cUnic.  These  results  sug- 
gest that  the  interpersonal  pressure  of  the  symptoms  of  psychotic  pa- 
tients does  not  differ  from  that  manifested  by  the  average  outpatient 
visitor,  and  (as  we  shall  see  in  the  next  chapter)  the  Level  I  fagade 
of  psychotics  is  significantly  more  hypernormal  than  that  of  out- 
patients who  go  into  psychotherapy. 

Level  II-C  Behavior  in  Three  Psychotic  Samples 

The  Level  II-C  self-diagnoses  of  the  hospitalized  patients  which 
are  presented  in  Table  33  present  an  even  more  interesting  pattern. 

TABLE  33 
Level  II-C  Diagnoses  of  46  Patients  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples 


Sample 

N 

Level  II-C  Diagnosis 
12       3       4       5      6      7      8 

Strong 

Loving 

1678 

Hostile 
Bitter 
2345 

State  Hospital  Sample  . . 

Clinic  Sample     

Private  Hospital  Sample  . 

Total  Sample   

6 

20 
20 

46 

2       3       0      0      0       10      0 
2       13       4      2       2       3       3 
4       12       116      14 

8      5       5       5       3       9      4      7 

3 
10 
15 

28 

3 

10 

5 

18 

There  is  a  definite  tendency  for  institutionalized  patients  to  see 
themselves  as  sweet,  hypernormal,  executive  individuals!  They  tend 
as  a  group  to  deny  hostile  or  weak  traits.  This  strongly  suggests  that 
many  psychotics  cling  to  a  conscious  self-perception  of  strength,  re- 
sponsibility, and  conventionality.  They  are  significantly  less  ahenated 
and  guilty  (at  Level  II-C)  than  outpatients  in  psychotherapy. 

Level  lll-T  Behavior  in  Three  Psychotic  Samples 

The  TAT  "hero"  indices  were  calculated  for  38  patients  in  these 
studies  and  then  plotted  on  the  diagnostic  grid.  Table  34  presents 
the  Level  III-T  diagnoses  for  the  three  samples. 


358         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

TABLE  34 

Level  III-T  Diagnoses  of  38  Patients  in  the  Three  Psychotic  Samples 


Sample 

N 

Level  III-T  Diagnosis 
12       3       4      5       6      7       8 

Strong 

Loving 

1678 

HostUe 
Bitter 
2345 

State  Hospital  Sample  . . 

20 
6 

12 

38 

0      0      2       0       12       0       1 
3       0      2       3       10      2       1 
3       3       5       3       2       2       0      2 

6       3       9      6      4      4      2       4 

3 
6 

7 

16 

3 
6 

Private  Hospital  Sample  . 
Total  Sample   

13 

22 

At  the  "preconscious"  level  these  patients  manifest  interpersonal 
themes  which  are  somewhat  different  from  their  overt  and  conscious 
presentations.  Sadistic  themes  are  the  most  common;  power  and  dis- 
trust are  the  two  next  most  frequent.  Whereas  the  psychotics  were 
significantly  more  bland  and  hypernormal  than  psychotherapy  out- 
patients in  conscious  self-description,  in  fantasy  they  are  significantly 
more  hostile. 

Multilevel  Personality  Patterns  of  the  Hospitalized  Samples 

It  is  now  possible  to  weave  together  the  results  from  the  three  levels 
of  personality  and  to  make  multilevel  summary  statements. 

At  the  symptomatic  level  (Level  I-M)  the  psychotics  equal 
psychiatric  clinic  outpatients  in  the  presentation  of  symptoms.  At 
the  level  of  conscious  self-diagnosis  a  larger  percentage  of  psychotics 
claim  strength  and  conventional  normality  than  do  outpatients.  At  the 
level  of  "preconscious"  fantasy,  however,  the  psychotics  are  more 
bitter  and  hostile  than  the  outpatients. 

These  data  have  suggested  the  following  hypothesis.  Many  psy- 
chotics show  highly  conflicted  personality  patterns.  Their  overt  se- 
curity operations  emphasize  strength  and  normality,  while  their  under- 
lying feelings  involve  sadism  and  bitterness.  Many  psychotics  cling 
desperately  to  a  conscious  fa9ade  of  conventionality  and  self-confi- 
dence in  the  teeth  of  their  underlying  feelings  of  rage  and  frustration. 

The  presenting  operations  of  many  neurotics,  on  the  contrary, 
stress  overt  passivity  and  bitterness,  while  their  underlying  feelings 
involve  stronger  and  more  affiliative  feelings. 

The  over-all  impression  obtained  from  these  studies  is  that  the 
process  of  hospitalization  involves  different  factors.  In  looking  over 
the  multilevel  diagnostic  codes  for  the  individual  patients,  it  is  apparent 
that  many  patients  are  institutionalized  because  they  are  immobilized 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS 


359 


by  anxiety,  distrust,  guilt,  and  helplessness  at  all  three  levels.  These 
would  be  clinically  labeled  anxiety-panic  states,  schizoid  conditions, 
psychotic  conditions,  or  psychotic  depressions.  Another  larger  group 
of  hospitalized  patients  try  to  maintain  a  fa9ade  of  strength  and  to 
cover  up  intense  underlying  feelings  of  rage  and  bitterness.  The  for- 
mer group  probably  includes  suicidal  risks,  w^ithdraw^n  and  apathetic 
operations.  The  latter  group  are  usually  called  paranoid.  Different 
therapeutic  implications  exist  for  these  two  broad  groups  of  insti- 
tutionalized cases. 

Implications  of  the  Multilevel  Patterns  of  Psychotic  Patients 

A  multilevel  system  of  personality  throws  into  clear  relief  the  con- 
flicts which  exist  in  a  patient's  character  structure.  We  have  just  re- 
viewed two  sets  of  evidence  suggesting  that  many  psychotics  and  pre- 
psychotics  present  a  two-layer  facade  of  strength  and  normality 
covering  intense  feelings  of  rage  and  despair.  There  are  several  im- 
plications. 

The  first  concerns  the  therapeutic  handling  of  psychotics.  Most 
hospitalized  patients  are  institutionalized  because  they  have  exhibited 
unconventional  behavior  which  frightens  or  alienates  others.  They 
are  seen  by  others  as  crazy,  psychotic,  disturbed. 

Many  of  these  patients,  however,  see  themselves  quite  differently 
— they  strive  to  present  themselves  as  confident  and  responsible.  It 
seems  clear  that  anyone  who  attempts  to  establish  constructive  com- 
munication with  this  kind  of  psychotic  must  pay  respect  to  the  fa9ade 
of  normality.  The  overt  operations  must  always  be  understood  and 
classified  before  therapy  can  deal  with  underlying  feelings.  Patients 
(in  the  cUnic  or  in  the  hospital)  who  present  a  fagade  of  strength 
tend  to  be  poorly  motivated  for  psychotherapy — since  treatment 
threatens  their  security  operations.  Extended  and  painstaking  pre- 
liminary procedures  (educational  talks,  discussions  in  which  the 
therapist  stays  on  the  side  of  the  ego)  may  be  necessary  to  prepare 
such  patients  for  conventional  psychotherapy.  If  these  are  short- 
circuited  and  an  attempt  is  made  to  plunge  the  patient  into  therapy  a 
disastrous  communication  situation  develops — in  which  the  therapist 
acts  as  though  the  patient  needs  treatment  and  the  patient  thinks  and 
acts  on  the  premise  that  he  does  not. 

This  multilevel  psychotic  profile  has  implications  for  the  use  of 
psychological  tests.  Many  psychologists  have  used  Level  I  and  II 
instruments  in  testing  institutionahzed  psychotics  and  have  been  dis- 
appointed in  finding  that  the  patients  appear  normal.  The  allegation 
has  been  made  that  the  MMPI  is  invahd  because  it  often  reveals  psy- 


360         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

chotics  as  having  normal  profiles.  Much  confusion  and  damage  has 
been  caused  by  researchers  who  have  administered  tests  to  patients 
with  a  unilevel  point  of  view. 

If  test  responses  are  viewed  as  interpersonal  communications  be- 
tween the  patient  and  the  psychologist  and  if  a  multilevel  approach  is 
maintained,  then  the  issue  of  validity  becomes  clarified.  If  a  psychotic 
produces  a  low  MMPI  profile  and  describes  himself  on  questionnaires 
as  nonsymptomatic,  this  does  not  invalidate  the  tests.  On  the  con- 
trary, these  results  provide  most  useful  information.  They  tell  the 
tester  that  the  patient  is  attempting  to  maintain  a  fa9ade  of  normality, 
that  he  wants  to  be  seen  as  healthy  and  nonneurotic.  The  conception 
of  levels  enters  here.  The  sophisticated  diagnostician  will  proceed  to 
administer  tests  which  tap  other  levels.  He  will  compare  the  overt 
and  conscious  "normal"  operations  with  Level  I  reports  from  observ- 
ers that  the  patient  acts  hostile,  or  with  Level  III  materials  indicating 
that  intense  private  feelings  of  distrust  exist.  The  bland  fagade  is 
seen  to  cover  paranoid  hostility  or  psychotic  despair. 

The  same  unilevel  error  has  been  made  by  research  psychologists 
who  have  attempted  to  demonstrate  that  the  TAT  fantasy  story  test 
is  invalid  because  it  does  not  differentiate  between  neurotics  and 
psychotics.  It  is  very  possible  that  a  sample  of  psychotics  will  show 
no  more  hostility  and  weakness  in  their  TAT  stories  than  neurotics. 
If  these  investigators  had  gone  on  to  collect  measures  at  the  overt 
levels,  they  would  have  been  surprised  to  discover  that  more  psy- 
chotics covered  their  fantasy  behavior  with  a  fa9ade  of  normality 
than  did  neurotics.  The  comparison  between  any  two  clinical  groups 
cannot  be  made  at  a  single  level  of  personality  without  confusion  or 
incomplete  results. 

The  multilevel  pattern  which  we  have  found  to  characterize  many 
psychotics  has  implications  for  evaluating  outpatients  for  psycho- 
therapy. Ad  any  patients  are  seen  for  intake  evaluation  in  the  Kaiser 
Foundation  clinic  who  manifest  the  multilevel  pattern  of  overt  con- 
ventionality with  underlying  sadism  or  distrust.  There  are  dozens  of 
such  multilevel  diagnoses,  e.g.,  773,  884,  173,  etc.  This  is  a  prepsy- 
chotic  pattern.  These  patients,  many  of  whom  are  presenting  psycho- 
somatic symptoms,  are  never  assigned  routinely  to  therapy  or  analysis. 
In  many  cases  they  are  given  the  opportunity  to  "repress  out"  of 
therapy  or  are  seen  in  supportive  counseling.  If  a  patient  with  a  pre- 
psychotic  multilevel  diagnosis  is  assigned  to  treatment,  the  therapist 
is  warned  to  watch  for  signs  of  anxiety.  The  question  is  posed:  can 
this  patient  tolerate  conscious  awareness  of  his  underlying  feelings? 
Reactions  to  the  earliest  interpretations  should  be  observed  to  see  if 
psychotic  trends  or  "flights  into  health"  are  developing. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  361 

Case  Illustrations  of  Six  Psychotic  Patients 

In  order  to  illustrate  the  use  of  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  sys- 
tem in  the  psychiatric  hospital,  we  shall  now  present  the  test  results 
and  clinical  histories  of  the  six  patients  from  the  state  hospital  sample. 

In  each  illustration  we  shall  consider  first  a  brief  case  history  and  a 
description  of  the  patient's  behavior  in  the  group.  The  interpersonal 
diagnostic  report  will  follow.  It  will  be  possible  to  compare  the  test 
results  with  the  way  the  patient  behaved  both  outside  the  hospital 
and  in  his  therapy  group. 

THE  WANDERER:  DIAGNOSIS  616  3 

(1)  Clinical  Data.  This  42-year-old  male  patient  was  born  in  a 
rural  Midwestern  state.  His  history  is  one  of  marginal  social  adjust- 
ment, marked  by  many  arrests  for  drunkenness,  molesting  children, 
vagrancy,  and  nomadism.  He  was  committed  to  this  hospital  shortly 
after  his  arrival  in  the  community  by  freight  train.  At  the  time  of  his 
hospitalization  the  patient  heard  voices  directed  by  the  church,  felt 
that  the  cabin  in  which  he  resided  was  wired  with  microphones  to 
find  out  his  thoughts,  felt  that  medicine  was  poisoned.  He  impressed 
the  examining  physician  as  "friendly  but  shy."  The  diagnosis  ad- 
vanced was  schizophrenic  reaction,  paranoid  type,  in  a  constitution- 
ally inadequate  individual  associated  with  chronic  alcoholism.  Electro- 
shock  therapy  was  initiated  shortly  after  the  patient's  admission  to  the 
hospital,  and  during  this  course  of  treatment  the  patient  was  involved 
in  numerous  "special  incident"  reports  and  as  a  consequence  was  fre- 
quently placed  in  seclusion  and  restraint.  After  twenty-four  electro- 
shock  treatments,  the  patient  was  transferred  to  an  open  ward  where 
he  had  ground  privileges.  He  was  a  member  of  the  therapy  group 
for  four  months.  Four  months  after  the  group  terminated  the  pa- 
tient's paranoid  symptoms  returned,  and  he  was  transferred  to  a 
closed  ward  where  a  second  series  of  electro-shock  treatments  was  be- 
gun. After  four  such  treatments  there  was  a  moderate  improvement 
and  electro-shock  therapy  was  discontinued.  At  last  report  the  patient 
was  doing  well  and  has  had  his  ground  privileges  restored. 

(2)  Group  Behavior.  The  patient  missed  a  few  of  the  early  meet- 
ings of  the  group  because  of  his  inability  to  remember  days  of  the 
week.  He  knew,  for  example,  that  meetings  were  held  each  Wednes- 
day at  9:00  a.m.,  but  did  not  know  when  it  was  Wednesday.   The 

^^  Certain  changes  in  peripheral  details  have  been  made  in  these  case  histories  in 
order  to  insure  anonymity.  An  attempt  to  preserve  the  essential  quahty  of  the  case 
history  has  been  made.  The  descriptive  titles  for  each  patient  were  supplied  by  the 
clinician  who  conducted  the  psychotherapy  group. 


362         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

patient's  speech,  especially  in  early  meetings,  was  circumstantial  and 
rambling;  his  manner  was  vague,  nebulous,  and  cloudy.  He  seemed 
uncertain  of  his  identity  and  seemed  to  have  very  few  resources  in  the 
way  of  stable  and  enduring  personaUty  characteristics.  His  adjust- 
ment appeared  to  be  on  a  day  to  day  and  even  on  a  minute  to  minute 
basis. 

At  about  the  seventh  meeting  of  the  group  the  patient  began  to 
change.  He  took  a  more  active  part  in  group  discussions,  no  longer 
missed  meetings,  and  displayed  obvious  interest  in  the  responses  and 
reactions  of  the  others  in  the  group.  Although  profoundly  amnesic 
to  many  episodes  in  his  past,  the  patient  seemed  to  be  trying  to  put 
the  pieces  together  and  re-establish  and  redefine  his  personality  out- 
lines. In  early  sessions  the  patient's  verbal  responses  were  character- 
istically platitudinous,  while  in  later  sessions  his  comments  seemed 
honest,  direct,  and  at  times,  poignant.  When  the  group  terminated, 
it  was  noted  that  while  the  patient  was  considerably  improved,  no 
change  in  his  hospital  status  was  recommended. 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  at  Level  I-M 
presents  as  mildly  depressed — somewhat  despondent.  He  does  not 
stress  his  symptoms,  does  not  attempt  to  make  an  extremely  sick  or 
neurotic  impression.  He  is  diagnosed  as  a  moderate  phobic  or  de- 
pendent personality  at  this  level.  This  seems  to  fit  the  clinician's  im- 
pression of  his  "friendly  but  shy"  approach. 

In  his  conscious  self-description  he  presents  a  mixed  picture.  He 
denies  hostility.  He  admits  to  some  passivity  but  also  claims  inde- 
pendence and  strength.  He  is  very  close  to  his  ego  ideal — indicating 
that  he  is  self-satisfied,  pleased  with  his  personality,  and  not  motivated 
for  psychotherapy. 

His  "preconscious"  hero  themes  stress  inordinate  passivity  and  feel- 
ings of  weakness. 

The  multilevel  pattern  thus  reveals  a  strong,  independent  fa9ade 
with  some  feehngs  of  depression — and  underlying  feelings  of  helpless- 
ness. His  basic  feelings  of  despair  and  weakness  are  expressed  in- 
directly in  his  symptoms  but  are  not  consciously  recognized.  These 
underlying  feelings  apparently  did  reach  expression  in  the  poignancy 
noted  in  later  sessions  of  the  group. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation.  This  patient  would 
not  be  considered  as  well-motivated  because  he  is  not  under  great 
symptomatic  pressure  (Level  I-M  =  moderate  6)  and  is  self-satisfied 
(Level  II-C  =  i). 

(b)  Prognosis.  Prognosis  is  complicated  by  the  underlying  feel- 
ings of  helplessness  and  dependence   (Level  III-T  =  6).    Male  pa- 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  363 

rients  whose  "preconscious"  themes  locate  in  this  octant  are  more 
difficult  to  treat  because  therapy  will  lead  to  the  expression  of  passive 
(and  usually  feminine)  emotions. 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  Level  I-M  and  Level 
II-C  measures  tend  to  predict  accurately  to  the  platitudinous  approach. 

THE  POIGNANT  ROMANTIC:  DIAGNOSIS  613 

(1)  Clinical  Data.  At  the  time  of  his  admission  to  the  hospital 
this  patient  was  a  42-year-old  white  married  male  who  spoke  in  an 
irrelevant,  illogical  manner  much  of  the  time.  He  showed  many  re- 
ligious delusions  and  was  depressed  and  agitated.  The  patient  had 
been  married  for  the  past  fourteen  years,  and  throughout  this  period 
showed  an  abnormally  strong  attachment  to  his  father.  He  has  fre- 
quently expressed  a  desire  to  help  his  father  at  the  expense  of  his  wife 
and  two  children,  who  were  often  in  dire  financial  straits.  He  would 
become  violently  angry  if  thwarted  in  his  desire  to  aid  his  father, 
and  on  one  occasion  knocked  out  several  of  his  wife's  teeth  when  she 
expostulated  with  him.  He  frequently  expressed  ideas  that  he  should 
make  his  living  as  a  writer,  although  he  has  had  nothing  published. 
In  the  two  months  immediately  prior  to  his  commitment  the  patient 
became  much  more  disturbed  and  confused.  He  would  preach  con- 
stantly and  incoherently,  stating  that  God  had  directly  communi- 
cated with  him. 

The  patient  was  given  electro-shock  therapy  and  made  an  im- 
mediate and  favorable  response  to  it.  After  six  treatments  the  patient 
was  much  improved.  He  became  a  member  of  the  therapy  group,  and 
after  the  group  terminated  the  patient  was  given  an  indefinite  leave 
of  absence  to  his  family.  On  subsequent  examinations  at  the  hospital 
the  patient  was  described  as  "sullen  and  aggressive"  by  the  examining 
physician,  but  there  had  been  no  relapse  of  sufficient  degree  to  war- 
rant hospitalization. 

(2)  Group  Behavior.  The  patient's  behavior  in  the  group  was 
characterized  chiefly  by  his  sober,  earnest  manner.  He  rarely  smiled, 
and  was  by  far  the  most  reflective  member  of  the  group.  From  the 
beginning,  the  patient's  contributions  to  the  group  discussion  M^ere 
relevant,  pertinent,  and  coherent,  with  no  evidence  of  the  psychotic 
manifestations  contained  in  the  commitment  report.  The  patient  was 
quite  self-punitive  in  presenting  his  problems  to  the  others  in  the 
group.  He  described  himself  as  a  failure  as  a  father,  as  a  husband,  as  a 
person,  as  a  writer.  He  had  much  to  say  about  how  he  always  felt 
he  should  be  a  writer;  how  he  always  admired  the  use  of  language 


364        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

and,  especially,  "big  words."  On  one  occasion  he  brought  a  collec- 
tion of  his  writings  to  the  group.  These  were  all  written  when  the 
patient  was  in  late  adolescence,  and  the  papers  on  which  they  were 
typed  were  crinkled  and  abused  by  age.  In  substance,  they  contained 
a  very  ponderous  philosophy  in  poetic  forms,  reminiscent  of  the  duller 
works  of  the  Victorian  period. 

The  patient  revealed  a  concern  for  the  problems  of  the  others  in 
the  group.  He  was  supportive  to  an  indiscriminate  degree,  and  seemed 
to  be  asking  for  support  when  he  chastised  himself  as  a  failure  before 
the  others.  Such  support  was  not  reciprocated  by  the  other  patients, 
however. 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  presents  at 
Level  I-M  a  mildly  depressed,  essentially  normal  picture.  (All  MMPI 
scales  are  below  70.)  He  is  definitely  not  attempting  to  impress 
others  as  a  sick,  nervous  person.  He  is  diagnosed  at  this  level  as  a  do- 
cile personality. 

In  his  conscious  self-descriptions  he  stresses  strength  and  hyper- 
normal  responsibility.  He  is  quite  close  to  his  ego  ideal — indicating 
self-satisfaction  and  no  awareness  of  any  need  to  change  his  personal- 
ity- .  ^ 

The  top  two  levels  thus  indicate  a  normal,  conventional  self-conn- 
dent  facade. 

At  the  level  of  fantasy  a  different  picture  develops.  Intense  feel- 
ings of  bitter  distrust  and  aggressive  power  are  expressed. 

The  three-level  pattern  involves  two  layers  of  normality  (he  is  a 
strong  man,  mildly  depressed)  covering  intense  sadistic  feelings. 

When  this  multilevel  pattern  is  compared  with  that  of  the  preced- 
ing patient,  we  observe  that  they  are  quite  similar  in  their  facades 
(both  61)  but  very  different  at  Level  III-T.  The  first  patient  ex- 
pressed helpless  fantasies,  which  we  related  to  his  poignancy.  The 
second  patient  manifests  bitter,  angry  feelings,  which  are  reflected  in 
the  sadistic  violence  reported  in  his  chnical  history  and,  perhaps,  in 
the  fact  that  the  group  responded  negatively  to  him. 

Once  again  we  see  a  common  psychotic  pattern  of  a  frail  fa9ade 
of  normality  conflicting  with  underlying  pathology. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation.  This  patient  would  be 
considered  unmotivated  because  of  the  symptom-free,  self-satisfied 
fagade. 

(b)  Prognosis.  The  prognosis  is  complicated  because  of  the  in- 
tense "preconscious"  feelings  of  hostility.  The  conflict  between  a 
bland  fagade  and  underlying  bitterness  (61^)  is  always  a  potentially 
explosive  one  and  difficult  to  treat  by  psychotherapy. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  365 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  three-level  diagnostic 
code  predicts  the  earnestness,  the  pedantry,  and  the  sullen  outbursts. 
It  does  not  pick  up  the  masochistic  self-derogation — unless  this  is 
interpreted  as  complaining  hostility. 

THE  NICE  GUY:  DIAGNOSIS  665 

( 1 )  Clinical  Data.  The  patient  was  committed  at  the  age  of  thirty- 
three  by  his  wife  and  his  mother  following  a  suicide  attempt  (sleeping 
pills).  The  patient  had  asked  his  wife  to  join  him  in  a  suicide  pact 
and  had  expressed  feelings  of  hopelessness  and  profound  despair  for 
the  few  weeks  immediately  preceding  his  hospitalization.  The  cUni- 
cal  decision  was  that  his  primary  diagnosis  was  a  reactive  depression, 
but  of  such  a  severe  nature  that  it  approached  psychotic  manifesta- 
tions. The  patient  adjusted  quite  well  to  the  hospital  milieu  from  the 
beginning.  He  was  assigned  to  an  open  ward  and  worked  days  in 
the  same  type  of  work  in  which  he  was  employed  before  hospitali- 
zation. No  somatic  therapy  was  deemed  necessary  or  advisable.  The 
patient  entered  the  therapy  group  and  remained  in  it  for  six  sessions. 
Then  he  left  the  hospital  without  permission.  The  patient's  elope- 
ment was  sudden  and  unexpected,  since  he  had  always  been  reluctant 
to  discuss  being  discharged  from  the  hospital.  The  patient  frequently 
stated  that  he  was  rather  afraid  to  leave  the  hospital  to  return  to  a 
world  in  which  all  sorts  of  terrible  things  could  happen  to  him.  Since 
the  patient's  unauthorized  departure,  no  word  has  been  received  by 
the  hospital  regarding  him. 

(2)  Group  Behavior.  Until  the  time  of  his  abrupt  departure  from 
the  hospital  the  patient  was  an  active  participant  in  the  therapy  group. 
A  quiet  man  by  inclination,  he  was  very  attentive  to  topics  of  discus- 
sion, listening  with  alertness  and  active  interest.  He  encouraged  other 
patients  to  discuss  their  problems  in  the  group  setting,  but  found  it  dif- 
ficult to  lead  the  way  by  using  his  own  case  as  an  example,  although 
this  was  his  expressed  intention.  The  patient  was  probably  in  a  better 
state  of  mental  health  than  any  other  group  member,  and  his  partici- 
pation directly  reflected  this.  When  another  patient  would  express 
delusional  material,  he  would  try  to  steer  the  conversation  into  more 
comfortable  channels.  He  was  well  liked  by  the  other  members  of 
the  group,  who  would  refer  to  him  as  a  "nice  guy."  The  patient  was 
discussed  more  freely  by  the  others  following  his  elopement  than  he 
was  during  the  period  in  which  he  was  an  active  group  member. 
Resentment  against  his  blandness  and  manner  of  departure  emerged, 
was  discussed,  and  was  related  to  the  patient's  attempts  to  control 
group  discussions  and  keep  them  on  a  "polite"  level. 


366        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  presents  a  pas- 
sive, dependent  picture  at  Level  I-M.  He  is  much  more  depressed, 
worried,  and  anxious  than  the  two  preceding  patients;  that  is  to  say, 
he  is  much  more  neurotic  at  the  symptomatic  level. 

His  conscious  self-perceptions  center  around  weakness,  docility, 
and  agreeability.  He  tends  to  be  hard  on  himself.  He  completely  de- 
nies any  strong,  generous  feelings  (which  were  claimed  by  the  two 
preceding  "sicker"  cases) . 

A  two-layer  fa9ade  of  extreme  passivity  and  docile  helplessness  is 
indicated. 

The  underlying  tests  emphasize  weakness,  guilt,  and  feelings  of  re- 
bellious bitterness. 

The  over-all  personality  structure  involves  three  layers  of  weak- 
ness. There  is  much  less  conflict  than  in  the  two  preceding  cases. 
This  patient  is  much  more  like  the  chronic  severe  neurotic  seen  (inter- 
minably) in  the  outpatient  clinic.  He  is  definitely  different  from  the 
rest  of  the  group.  He  is  the  only  patient  of  the  six  who  diagnoses  him- 
self (at  Level  II-C)  as  weak  and  needing  help.  He  is  the  only  mem- 
ber who  stresses  neurotic  symptoms — depression  and  anxiety.  When 
we  recall  the  therapist's  statement  that  this  patient  was  in  a  "better 
state  of  mental  health  than  any  other  group  member,"  it  becomes 
clear  that  internalization  and  expression  of  anxiety  is  a  salutary  security 
operation — a  protection  against  psychosis. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation.  This  patient  is  well- 
motivated  for  psychotherapy.  He  experiences  symptomatic  pressure. 
He  is  dissatisfied  with  his  personality  (c.f.  the  self  versus  ideal  dis- 
crepancy). 

(b)  Prognosis.  A  rigid  and  deep-seated  commitment  to  passive, 
masochistic  security  operations  (665)  suggests  a  slow  prognosis.  This 
patient  tends  to  avoid  (at  all  levels)  strong,  responsible  behavior. 
Negative  identifications  and  underlying  guilt  will  make  therapy  a  very 
long-term  proposition. 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  top  level  scores  (66) 
predict  a  docile,  conforming,  placating  fagade  and  an  avoidance  of 
hostile  relations.  They  correlate  with  the  clinician's  impression  of  a 
"nice  guy."  The  tests  ignominiously  fail  to  predict  his  going  AWOL. 
We  should  expect  masochistic  self-effacement.  The  TAT  does  pick 
up  some  rebelliousness  but  does  not  forecast  an  active  disaffiliation. 

There  is  an  interesting  side  issue  which  develops  from  this  case 
history.  This  patient  is  the  only  member  of  the  group  who  was  not 
diagnosed  as  schizophrenic.  He  was  given  the  label  severe  reactive 
depression.  This  patient  manifests  the  "weakest"  multilevel  pattern. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  367 

He  is  the  only  patient  in  the  group  who  is  dependent  or  masochistic 
at  all  three  levels  (665),  the  only  patient  who  does  not  claim  or  ex- 
press strength  at  some  level. 

This  patient  was  hospitaUzed  not  for  psychotic  symptoms,  but  for 
suicidal  depression.   No  delusional  or  paranoid  material  was  elicited. 

This  patient  does  not  fit  the  multilevel  pattern  of  the  paranoid 
group  of  five  patients  who  claim  normality  or  self-confidence  and 
repress  hostility.  He  stands  as  the  representative  of  a  second  multi- 
level type  which  is  often  seen  in  psychiatric  hospitals — patients  who 
are  crippled  and  incapacitated  by  a  solid  three-layer  structure  of 
despair,  helplessness,  and  masochism. 

CYNIC  AND  TOUGH  GUY:  DIAGNOSIS  126 

( 1 )  Clinical  Data.  This  42 -year-old  patient  was  born  in  Texas,  the 
youngest  of  fourteen  children.  He  has  resided  in  California  since 
1937,  and  his  present  hospitalization  began  in  1953.  The  patient  was 
previously  committed  to  this  hospital  in  1940,  when  he  was  diagnosed 
as  schizophrenic  reaction,  simple  type.  He  was  discharged  early  in 
1945  as  "recovered,"  but  was  recommitted  in  1953  upon  the  petition 
of  his  mother  and  sibhngs  as:  "confused  .  .  .  mumbles  to  himself 
.  .  .  frightens  neighbors  .  .  .  loud  screaming  .  .  .  abusive."  The 
patient  was  brought  to  the  hospital  with  a  black  eye  by  pohce,  who 
said  that  he  had  incurred  it  while  resisting  arrest.  The  patient  has  had 
many  altercations  with  the  law,  usually  occurring  on  occasions  when 
he  was  intoxicated  and/or  driving.  The  patient  has  never  married 
and  disclaims  any  close  attachments.  In  appearance  he  is  lanky,  dour, 
undernourished.  Throughout  his  hospitalization  the  patient  received 
no  somatic  or  psychotherapy  until  becoming  a  member  of  the  therapy 
group.  In  May,  1954,  the  patient  was  granted  a  town  pass  to  get  a 
job  with  leave-to-self  recommended  when  he  found  employment. 
The  patient  would  leave  the  grounds  early  in  the  morning  and  come 
back  to  the  hospital  to  sleep.  This  continued  until  July,  1954,  when 
the  patient  returned  to  the  hospital  in  an  intoxicated  condition  and 
abusive  manner.  He  was  transferred  to  a  closed  unit,  where  he  cur- 
rently receives  electro-shock  therapy. 

(2)  Group  Behavior.  The  patient  attended  all  the  group  therapy 
sessions  and  maintained  a  forthright  and  consistent  position  through- 
out their  course,  to  wit,  that  he  should  not  have  been  put  in  the  hos- 
pital, that  there  was  nothing  whatever  the  matter  with  him,  that  hos- 
pitalization was  more  suitable  to  the  needs  of  those  members  of  his 
family  who  had  him  committed.  The  patient  frequently  stated  that  if 


368         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

the  state  wanted  to  support  him  and  provide  him  with  an  easy  life  he 
had  no  objections  to  such  a  program.  He  was  an  active  member  of 
the  group,  but  formed  no  close  attachments  within  it.  He  often 
scoffed  at  the  remarks  of  the  other  group  members,  and  on  several 
occasions  shocked  the  other  patients  by  making  casual  and  crude  ob- 
servations on  sexual  topics.  His  personality  defenses  seemed  well- 
organized  at  all  times,  and  he  was  able  to  maintain  his  equilibrium 
when  pressured  by  other  patients  as  to  why  he  found  it  necessary  to 
be  such  a  "tough  guy"  at  all  times.  The  patient  responded  to  this  by 
saying  that  if  feelings  were  put  out  in  the  open  "somebody  would 
stomp  on  them."  This  type  of  stomping  was  frequently  demonstrated 
by  him  on  the  feelings  of  other  group  members.  At  the  termination 
of  the  group  self-leave  was  recommended,  since  he  was  functioning 
on  a  nonpsychotic  level. 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  at  Level  I-M 
manifests  a  strong,  unworried  front.  There  is  no  attempt  to  present 
as  a  sick  person — neurotic  symptoms  are  denied.  His  interpersonal 
diagnosis  at  this  level  is — autocratic  personality. 

The  underlying  tests  reveal  intense  feelings  of  weakness,  helpless- 
ness, and  dependence. 

A  fierce  conflict  exists  between  overt  toughness  and  "preconscious" 
passivity.  He  cannot  tolerate  awareness  of  his"  underlying  fear  and 
impotence.  He  attempts  to  maintain  strong  counterphobic  operations. 

This  multilevel  pattern  (126)  seems  to  fit  the  clinical  picture.  He 
was  able  to  express  in  group  therapy  his  need  to  be  strong  and  his 
anxieties  about  being  seen  as  weak.  He  apparently  was  willing  to  stay 
in  a  protected,  dependent  situation  in  the  hospital  (thus  satisfying  his 
underlying  passivity) ,  while  stoutly  maintaining  the  verbal  picture  of 
strength. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation.  This  patient  is  not  a 
candidate  for  psychotherapy.  His  counterphobic  operations  would  be 
threatened  by  the  implications  of  treatment.  He  has  no  conscious 
feehngs  of  anxiety  or  depression.  He  is  satisfied  with  his  adjustment 
(no  discrepancy  between  Level  II-C  and  Level  V-C). 

(b)  Prognosis.  The  therapeutic  outlook  for  the  126  personality 
type  is  guarded.  The  conflict  is  intense,  the  fagade  is  brittle  (the  un- 
derlying passivity  does  not  leak  through  in  the  form  of  symptoms  and 
is  completely  avoided  at  Level  II-C).  There  is  the  additional  factor 
that  underlying  passivity  often  leads  to  a  poor  prognosis.  The  patient 
is  warding  off  feelings  of  impotence  and,  in  many  cases,  feminine 
identification.   Unless  ego  strength  is  pronounced  (which  is  not  the 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  369 

case  with  this  patient),  recognition  of  the  underlying  feelings  will  be 
accompanied  by  intense  anxiety. 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  test  pattern  (12)  pre- 
dicts fairly  well  the  therapist's  descriptive  title  of  "cynic  and  tough 
guy." 


THE  COMMENTATOR:  DIAGNOSIS  228 

(1)  Clinical  Data.  This  patient  is  a  35-year-old  divorced  white 
male  who  was  committed  from  a  county  hospital.  For  about  two 
weeks  prior  to  commitment  the  patient  complained  of  a  dust  which 
kept  falling  from  the  ceiling  and  choking  him.  He  was  told  this  was 
"psycho  dust"  by  voices  which  he  was  unable  to  identify,  except  that 
he  thought  doctors  might  be  attempting  to  treat  him  from  "long  dis- 
tance." He  was  well  oriented  to  time,  place,  and  person.  He  dis- 
played no  gross  personality  disorganization.  For  this  reason  electro- 
shock  therapy  was  not  selected  as  a  proper  treatment  method,  and 
psychotherapy  was  recommended.  The  patient  was  transferred  to 
an  open  ward  and  became  a  member  of  the  therapy  group. 

The  patient  was  born  in  Alabama  and  resided  in  the  southern  re- 
gion of  the  United  States  until  he  entered  the  army  during  World 
War  II.  Since  his  youth  the  patient  has  been  a  heavy  drinker  and  was 
frequently  arrested  when  intoxicated.  On  several  occasions  he  ex- 
perienced delirium  tremens  and  was  admitted  to  an  Alabama  state 
hospital  until  they  subsided.  He  performed  quite  well  in  the  armed 
forces  and  was  a  technical  sergeant  at  the  time  of  his  discharge.  Since 
leaving  the  service,  the  patient  has  adjusted  on  quite  a  marginal  basis, 
shifting  from  job  to  job  and  town  to  town,  drinking  heavily  and  work- 
ing as  a  seasonal  unskilled  laborer. 

Following  group  therapy  the  patient  was  being  considered  for  self- 
leave  and  eventual  discharge  when  he  was  transferred  to  a  Veterans 
Administration  hospital. 

( 2 )  Group  Behavior.  The  patient  attended  all  group  sessions,  and 
although  he  never  presented  any  problems  of  his  own  for  group  dis- 
cussion, he  was  a  very  active  participant.  He  speaks  in  a  rather  drawl- 
ing Southern  accent  with  a  dry  humor  which  was  especially  effective 
because  of  the  apt  sense  of  timing  that  he  displayed.  He  was  liked 
and  respected  by  the  other  group  members,  who  referred  to  him  as 
"intelligent."  He  never  put  forth  any  facts  about  himself  or  any  prob- 
lems except  for  some  very  superficial  facts  regarding  past  education, 
vocational  background,  etc.   His  chief  role  in  the  group  setting  was 


370 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


to  act  as  a  sort  of  commentator,  interpreting  the  remarks  of  the  others 
in  a  humorous  and  sometimes  penetrating  manner,  reminiscent  of  Will 
Rogers.  In  private  conversations  with  the  psychotherapist  the  patient 
expressed  some  anxiety  about  being  deported  to  a  state  hospital  in 
Alabama  where  his  family  lived,  but  he  did  not  discuss  this  or  any 
other  area  of  insecurity  in  the  group.  He  offered  general  comments 
and  advice  to  other  patients  in  the  group,  but  discouraged  any  in- 
quiries that  they  might  have  regarding  his  own  feelings. 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  presents  a  very 
mixed  picture  at  Level  I-M.  He  feels  depressed,  worried,  isolated,  and 
alienated.  He  thus  internalizes  his  problems  and  recognizes  emotional 
symptomology.  On  the  other  hand,  he  tends  to  emphasize  physical 
symptoms  and  bland  activity.  There  is,  moreover,  some  tendency  to 
maintain  a  conventional  denial  of  psychopathology.  An  intense  am- 
bivalence at  this  level  is  apparent.  The  tendencies  to  minimize  emo- 
tions and  to  maintain  strength  are  stronger  than  the  admission  of 
weakness.  He  is  diagnosed  at  Level  I-M  as  a  narcissistic  personality. 

In  his  self-perception  he  presents  himself  as  normal  and  self-confi- 
dent.  His  diagnosis  at  Level  II-C  is  competitive  personality. 

His  underlying  tests  are  also  conflicted,  expressing  strength  and 
conformity. 

The  indices  from  all  five  interpersonal  tests  administered  to  this 
patient  fall  in  a  narrow  sector  of  the  diagnostic  grid — reflecting  self- 
confidence  and  strength.  There  is  some  emotional  symptomology,  but 
this  is  minimized  by  the  effort  (apparent  at  Levels  II  and  III)  to  act 
as  a  conventional,  executive  person. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation.  His  overt  security  op- 
erations tend  to  make  him  an  unmotivated  patient.  He  sees  himself  as 
close  to  his  ego  ideal.  He  admits  to  some  emotional  symptomology 
but  the  repressive  externalizing  tendencies  are  considerably  stronger. 
He  denies  wanting  or  needing  help. 

(b)  Prognosis.  This  patient  exerts  a  rigid  control  over  his  inter- 
personal behavior.  Although  he  is  riddled  with  symptoms  and  rele- 
gated by  society  to  a  psychiatric  hospital,  he  still  maintains  a  four- 
level  structure  of  strength  and  conventional  leadership.  This  rigidity 
may  give  him  a  certain  stability  and  make  it  possible  for  him  to 
function  more  adequately  than  the  other  group  members — but  the 
inflexibihty  means  that  any  major  change  is  not  to  be  expected. 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  solid  multilevel  com- 
mitment to  self-confident,  managerial  operations  (228)  predicts  ac- 
curately his  role  in  the  group. 


INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PSYCHOTICS  371 

THE  AVENGER:  DIAGNOSIS  123 

(1)  Clinical  Data.  This  parient  is  a  41-year-old  white  male  who 
was  born  in  Nevada  but  lived  most  of  his  life  in  California,  where  he 
obtained  a  university  degree  in  business  administration.  He  then  en- 
tered the  employ  of  a  major  oil  company  and  traveled  extensively  in 
its  service.  In  1941  while  on  foreign  duty  he  attempted  suicide  and 
was  returned  to  the  United  States  but  not  hospitalized.  In  1951  he 
was  briefly  hospitalized  because  of  a  schizophrenic  episode.  He  re- 
sponded rapidly  to  electro-shock  therapy  and  insulin  and  was  dis- 
charged. 

At  the  time  of  his  commitment  the  patient  was  married  and  the 
father  of  two  children.  He  was  extremely  bitter  about  his  hospitaliza- 
tion and  very  grandiose  in  his  rationalizations.  He  was  persecuted, 
drugged,  perhaps  poisoned,  spied  upon;  the  victim  of  greedy  relatives 
and  incompetent  doctors.  He  made  numerous  threats  and  promises 
of  revenge  and  retribution.  Electro-shock  therapy  had  no  noticeable 
effect  and  was  discontinued.  The  patient  became  a  member  of  the 
therapy  group  when  it  was  organized,  but  refused  to  continue  after 
the  second  week.  Shortly  afterwards  electro-shock  therapy  was  re- 
sumed, and  this  time  he  showed  improvement.  He  was  transferred 
to  the  hospital  annex,  where  his  recovery  progressed  rapidly.  He  was 
described  as  a  willing,  cheerful,  cooperative  worker  at  the  time  of  his 
release  on  indefinite  leave  of  absence. 

( 2  )  Group  Behavior.  The  patient  came  to  the  first  meeting  of  the 
group  and  was  by  far  its  most  active  member.  He  assumed  leadership 
of  the  group,  questioned  other  patients,  and  steered  the  topics  of  con- 
versation to  world  affairs  such  as  A-bomb  strategy,  etc.  The  other  pa- 
tients in  the  group  offered  no  overt  objections  to  his  taking  charge, 
but  the  patient  felt  that  the  therapist  should  have  asserted  himself 
more  than  he  did.  During  the  second  meeting  of  the  group  the  pa- 
tient announced  that  he  was  "resigning"  from  it.  He  gave  as  his 
reasons  the  incompetence  of  the  therapist,  the  poor  quality  of  the 
hospital  staff,  the  fact  that  he  had  been  receiving  poisoned  cigarettes, 
etc.  The  therapist  encouraged  him  to  remain  in  the  group,  but  could 
only  elicit  from  him  a  promise  to  defer  his  decision  until  the  next 
week.  The  patient  never  came  to  any  subsequent  meeting,  although  he 
was  told  that  he  was  welcome  to  do  so. 

(3)  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report.  This  patient  presents  him- 
self at  Level  I-M  as  a  forceful,  executive,  active  person  completely 
free  from  any  psychological  symptoms.  He  does  not  want  to  be  seen 


372         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

as  sick  or  isolated  but,  on  the  contrary,  stresses  his  mental  health  and 
his  conventional  success.  His  diagnosis  at  this  level  is  autocratic  per- 
sonality. 

His  self-perception  duplicates  almost  exactly  his  symptomatic  im- 
pact— although  there  is  more  emphasis  on  independence.  His  diag- 
nosis at  Level  II-C  is  narcissistic  personality. 

His  "preconscious"  themes  are  loaded  with  superiority,  rage,  and 
bitterness.  He  is  diagnosed  at  Level  III-T  as  a  sadistic  personality. 

(4)  Clinical  Implications,  (a)  Motivation,  This  patient  is  com- 
pletely unmotivated  for  psychotherapy.  He  cannot  stand  any  close  or 
dependent  relationships  and  maintains  strong,  defiant  behavior  at  all 
levels.  He  is,  of  course,  very  self-satisfied  and  has  no  apparent  desire 
to  change  himself. 

(b)  Prognosis.  The  test  pattern  predicts  that  he  will  not  change. 
This  disagrees  with  the  clinical  history  which  describes  his  cheerful 
cooperative  recovery.  This  discrepancy  may  be  a  test  miss.  There 
is  some  possibility  that  his  later  conforming  behavior  is  a  deliberate 
repressive  maneuver  to  obtain  discharge. 

(c)  Predicted  interpersonal  behavior.  The  multilevel  diagnosis 
(123)  perfectly  predicts  the  sequence  of  his  behavior  in  the  group — 
bossiness  followed  by  an  angry  departure.  It  does  not  predict  his  re- 
covery— but  there  is  some  possibility  that  he  has  temporarily  and  de- 
liberately changed  his  tactics,  not  his  personality  structure. 


24 


Interpersonal  Diagnosis  in  Medical 
Practice:  Psychosomatic  Personality  Types 


The  interpersonal  system  of  diagnosis  has  been  developed  and  vali- 
dated by  an  outpatient  psychiatric  clinic.  In  the  last  chapter  we  have 
reported  some  applications  of  the  diagnostic  systems  in  an  inpatient 
setting — the  psychiatric  hospital.  The  locale  of  investigation  now 
moves  again — this  time  to  the  office  of  the  medical  practitioner.  Are 
there  typical  multilevel  personality  patterns  characteristic  of  the  dif- 
ferent psychosomatic  conditions?  If  so,  what  are  the  functional  im- 
plications of  these  personaUty  factors?  What  do  they  mean  to  the 
internist  who  deals  with  these  patients  for  treatment? 

Several  intensive  empirical  studies  have  been  made  which  provide 
tentative  answers  to  these  questions.  Several  hundred  psychosomatic 
patients  have  been  diagnosed  by  the  interpersonal  system.  The  re- 
sults indicate  that  the  psychosomatic  conditions  investigated  have 
typical  personality  correlates.  The  psychosomatic  symptom  groups 
with  which  we  have  been  mainly  concerned  are:  duodenal  ulcer,  es- 
sential hypertension,  obesity,  and  dermatitis  conditions  (of  unknown 
physical  etiology).  Samples  of  medical  controls  have  been  collected 
to  compare  with  the  psychosomatic  groups.  We  have  also  followed 
the  practice  of  comparing  the  behavior  of  psychosomatic  patients  with 
neurotic  and  psychotic  patients  and  a  psychiatric  clinic  admission  sam- 
ple. These  studies  have  two  purposes:  (1)  to  throw  light  on  the 
factors  which  differentiate  among  the  four  major  symptom  types:  nor- 
mals, psychosomatics,  neurotics,  and  psychotics;  (2)  to  test  the  va- 
lidity of  the  interpersonal  system,  i.e.,  to  see  if  the  system  differen- 
tiates these  groups  at  the  several  levels  of  personality. 

This  chapter  reviews  the  psychosomatic  research  which  has  been 
executed  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  project.  At  this  point  the  results 
are  far  from  definitive.  They  are  being  presented  here  not  to  prove 

373 


374        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

anything  about  the  psychosomatic  groups  or  to  claim  that  psychoso- 
matic patients  can  be  diagnosed  by  means  of  personality  tests.  The 
study  of  any  personality  or  symptom  type  is  an  enormously  com- 
plex task  involving  multilevel  patterns  on  large  samples.  In  the  case  of 
psychosomatic  groups,  the  external  criteria  themselves  present  taxing 
medical  diagnostic  problems. 

The  following  studies  are,  therefore,  an  attempt  to  illustrate  the 
interpersonal  diagnostic  system  in  action  on  research  questions.  The 
multilevel  analysis  clarifies  certain  issues  and  raises  new  hypotheses. 

This  chapter  is  outlined  as  follows:  First,  the  ten  samples  are  de- 
scribed; then,  the  behavior  of  each  symptom  group  at  Level  I-M  is 
presented  and  the  results  discussed;  then,  the  same  ten  groups  are 
compared  at  Level  II-C.  This  is  followed  by  the  results  at  Level  III-T. 
With  the  multilevel  pattern  of  each  group  in  hand,  it  will  then  be 
possible  to  present  the  typical  personality  structure  and  the  nuclear 
conflicts  of  each  of  the  ten  important  symptom  groups. 

The  implications  for  medical  and  psychiatric  handHng  of  these 
cases  will  be  included  in  these  discussions. 


Descrii 

mon  of  Samples 

The 

ten  symptom  groups  to  be  described  in  this  chapter  are: 

Code 

N 

Sample^ 

U 

41 

1.  Duodenal  ulcer 

H 

49 

2.  Essential  hypertension 

O 

98 

3.  Obesity 

ND 

31 

4.  Overtly  neurotic  dermatitis 

SID 

57 

5.  Self-inflicted  dermatitis 

UD 

73 

6.  Unanxious  dermatitis 

C 

38 

7.  Normals   (medical  controls  who  were  not  seeking 
help  for  emotional  or  psychosomatic  symptoms) 

N 

67 

8.  Neurotics  (in  psychotherapy) 

P 

28 

9.  Psychotics  (committed  to  state  hospitals) 

A 

207 
689 

10.  Random  sample  of  psychiatric  clinic  admissions 

The  Duodenal  Ulcer  Sample^ 

There  are  41  patients  in  the  Duodenal  Ulcer  Sample.  Of  these,  32 
are  males  and  9  females.  These  patients  were  referred  from  the  gastro- 
intestinal clinic  by  internists  using  these  criteria:  (1)  positive  X-ray 
diagnosis  of  either  gastric  or  duodenal  ulcer,  (2)  the  absence  of  any 

*  These  N's  refer  to  the  total  sample.  The  A7's  for  any  level  are  slightly  different 
because  some  patients  failed  to  take  tests  at  all  three  levels. 

^The  author  is  grateful  to  P.  Raimondi,  M.D.,  for  providing  the  criteria  and  se- 
lecting the  patients  for  the  ulcer  sample. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  375 

other  explanation  for  the  finding,  and  (3)  typical  pain-food-relief 
sequence  of  symptoms. 

These  patients  had  not  requested  a  psychiatric  referral;  they  were 
selected  on  the  basis  of  their  symptoms  for  research  investigation. 
These  selection  factors  must  be  taken  into  account.  The  fact  that 
these  patients  were  not  involved  in  a  psychiatric  clinic  referral  may 
subtly  influence  their  test  responses.  In  all  the  findings  reported  be- 
low, it  should  be  remembered  that  the  ulcer  sample  was  tested  under 
circumstances  different  from  the  psychiatric  clinic  samples. 

The  Essential  Hypertension  Sample^ 

There  are  49  hypertensive  patients  in  this  study.  Of  these,  27  are 
men  and  22  women.  These  patients  were  referred  from  the  cardio- 
vascular clinic  with  the  diagnosis  of  essential  hypertension  based  on 
elaborate  criteria  being  used  for  a  simultaneous  study  of  hypertensive 
diseases.  In  general,  they  are  patients  below  the  age  of  forty-five, 
with  blood  pressure  consistently  in  excess  of  145  mm  Hg  systolic  and 
90  mm  Hg  diastolic,  who,  by  means  of  kidney  function  tests,  ephine- 
phrin  neutralization  tests,  etc.,  were  found  to  have  no  discernible  cause 
for  their  elevated  blood  pressure. 

The  hypertensive  patients  were  selected  for  a  research  study  and 
were  not  self -referred  for  psychiatric  evaluation.  These  selective  fac- 
tors may  have  influenced  their  attitude  toward  testing  and  therefore 
the  findings  should  be  interpreted  with  this  possibiUty  in  mind. 

The  Obesity  Sample'^ 

The  98  female  subjects  who  comprised  the  Obesity  Sample  were 
part  of  a  large-scale  study  of  obesity.  The  tests  were  administered 
before  and  after  participation  in  discussion  groups  which  lasted  for 
about  four  months.  These  subjects  were  self-referred  for  weight  re- 
duction and  did  not  come  for  a  psychiatric  evaluation.  The  fact  that 
they  were  not  seen  in  a  psychiatric  setting  may  have  influenced  their 
responses.  The  fact  that  the  entire  sample  is  comprised  of  women  is 
another  serious  limitation.  In  all  other  samples  studied,  males  are 
stronger  (but  not  to  a  significant  degree)  at  the  fa9ade  levels  than 
females.  This  factor  should  be  taken  into  account  when  the  data  are 
considered. 

^  The  author  is  grateful  to  A.  A.  Bolomey,  M.D.,  for  defining  the  cnteria  and  se- 
lecting the  patients  for  the  hypertensive  sample. 

*  The  MMPI  and  interaction  data  for  the  obesity  sample  were  taken  from  the 
Herrick  Hospital  Research  Project  on  obesity.  This  research,  supported  by  Public 
Health  funds,  has  studied  several  factors— dietary,  physiological,  and  psychological— 
w^hich  may  be  related  to  obesity.  The  psychological  factors  in  the  Herrick  study 
have  been  investigated  by  Robert  Suczek,  Ph.D.,  whose  theoretical  and  practical  con- 
tributions to  our  work  have  been  most  valuable. 


376        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

The  Three  Dermatitis  Samples^ 

The  dermatitis  samples  comprise  161  subjects  of  which  67  are  males 
and  94  females.  These  patients  were  taken  from  the  private  practice 
of  a  dermatologist  and  were  tested  in  his  office  by  a  secretary  who 
was  trained  in  the  necessary  psychometric  methods. 

The  criterion  used  to  select  patients  for  the  dermatitis  sample  was 
the  presence  of  a  skin  symptom  for  which  there  exists  no  established 
physiological  etiology.  The  specific  symptomatic  categories  which 
made  up  the  dermatitis  sample  were  the  following: 

Acne  Hyperhidrotic  eczema 

Psoriasis  Alopecia  areata 

Seborrheic  dermatitis  Urticaria 

Atopic  dermatitis  Acne  rosacea 

Eczematous  dermatitis  Lupus  erythematosus 

Pruritis  Herpes  simplex 

Otitis  externa  Warts 
Neurotic  excoriations 

This  sample  was  selected  under  circumstances  somewhat  different 
from  any  other  sample.  Subtle  additudinal  factors  may  have  partially 
determined  the  results;  consequently,  the  findings  should  be  con- 
sidered with  this  caution  in  mind. 

Examination  of  the  multilevel  patterns  of  the  dermatitis  patients 
revealed  that  considerable  differences  exist  among  the  different  symp- 
tomatic groups.  For  example,  the  acne  sample  presents  differently  at 
all  levels  from  the  pruritis  sample.  It  seems  clear  that  skin  symptoms 
do  not  manifest  one  personality  syndrome.  The  skin  is,  of  course, 
mediated  by  a  complex  set  of  physiological  systems  and  is,  in  addi- 
tion, vulnerable  to  more  external  stimuli  (e.g.,  self-inflicted  excoria- 
tions) than  any  other  organ  system. 

For  these  reasons  it  seemed  advisable  to  divide  the  dermatitis  sample 
into  three  relatively  homogeneous  subgroups.  These  are  tentatively 
labeled  the  Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Sample,  the  Self-inflicted 
Dermatitis  Sample,  and  the  Unanxious  Dermatitis  Sample, 

The  symptomatic  subgroups  which  comprise  the  Overtly  Neurotic 
Dermatitis  Sample  are  acne,  psoriasis,  and  seborrheic  dermatitis.  The 
psychological  characteristics  defining  this  group  are:  anxiety  and  de- 
pression.  The  physiological  criteria  are  not  well  defined,  but  there 

'  The  dermatitis  and  neurodermatitis  studies  reported  in  this  chapter  are  part  of  a 
large-scale  study  of  emotional  factors  in  dermatologic  patients  being  conducted  by 
Herbert  Lawrence,  M.D.,  Edward  Weinshel,  M.D.,  and  the  author.  The  criteria  for 
defining  these  conditions  were  supplied  and  the  selection  of  cases  was  accomplished  by 
Dr.  Lawrence. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  377 

seems  to  be  a  greater  involvement  of  the  sweat  or  oil  apparatus.  There 
are  3 1  patients  in  this  subsample:  20  females  and  1 1  males. 

The  Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Sample  includes  the  following  symp- 
tomatic subgroups:  atopic  dermatitis,  eczematous  dermatitis,  pruritis, 
otitis  externa,  and  neurotic  excoriations.  This  group  is  characterized 
psychologically  by  less  anxiety  and  depression  and  considerably  more 
underlying  sado-masochism  than  the  Overtly  Neurotic  Sample.  The 
dermatological  criteria  which  define  this  group  are  vague  but  would 
include  itching,  scratching,  and  more  self-inflicted  damage  to  the  skin. 
This  subsample  includes  57  patients:  33  females  and  24  males. 

The  Unanxious  Dermatitis  Sample  contains  the  following  symptom 
groups:  hyperhydrotic  eczema,  alopecia  areata,  urticaria,  acne  rosacea, 
lupus  erythematous,  herpes  simplex,  and  warts.  The  subsample  is 
characterized  (psychologically)  by  a  hypernormal  facade  with  under- 
lying sado-masochistic  trends.  The  physiological  criteria  defining  this 
group  are  quite  vague  but  would  include  circulatory  and  virus  factors. 
This  sample  contains  73  subjects:  41  females  and  32  males. 

The  following  code  designations  have  been  assigned  to  the  derm- 
atitis samples: 

ND  =  3 1     Overtly  neurotic  skin  patients 
SID  =  57     Self-inflicted  skin  patients 
UD  =  73     Unanxious  skin  patients 

The  scores  for  each  of  these  subgroups  will  be  presented  in  the 
dermatitis  section  of  this  chapter. 

The  Normal  Control  Sample 

A  group  of  38  subjects,  21  male  and  17  female,  made  up  the  medi- 
cal control  sample.  These  subjects  were  patients  seen  in  the  derma- 
tologist's office  for  skin  lesions  for  which  a  definite  physiological 
(nonpsychosomatic)  cause  existed — industrial  dermatitis,  infections, 
skin  carcinomas,  etc.  These  patients  were  tested  in  the  same  manner 
as  the  dermatitis  sample  and  were  used  as  a  direct  control.  This  group 
stands  as  the  only  sample  for  which  there  is  no  apparent  psychiatric 
or  psychosomatic  involvement  and  is,  therefore,  designated  medical 
or  "normal"  control. 

The  Neurotic  Sample 

A  group  of  67  patients,  23  male  and  44  female,  who  had  entered 
and  remained  in  psychotherapy  at  an  outpatient  psychiatric  clinic 
comprise  the  Neurotic  Sample.  Of  all  the  patients  seen  for  intake 
valuation  at  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Clinic,  less  than  40  per  cent  go 
into  treatment.    These  tend  to  be  patients  who  recognize  and  ac- 


378        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

cept  the  need  for  treatment.  They  tend  to  manifest  openly  the  symp- 
toms of  anxiety,  fear,  depression,  isolation,  etc.  They  present  as  neu- 
rotics and  do  not  deny  emotional  symptoms  as  do  the  psychosomatic 
samples.  For  this  reason,  they  have  been  labeled  the  neurotic  or  ther- 
apy sample. 

Motivational  factors  may  have  influenced  their  test  results.  It  is 
likely  that  many  of  these  patients  were  strongly  desirous  of  therapy. 
They  may  have  slanted  their  test  responses  in  the  direction  of  ad- 
mitting a  greater  number  of  neurotic  symptoms.  The  findings  should 
be  studied  with  these  factors  in  mind. 

The  Psychotic  Sample 

The  Psychotic  Sample  is  composed  of  patients  who  were  com- 
mitted to  a  psychiatric  hospital  for  inpatient  custody  and  treatment. 
Six  of  these  subjects  (male)  were  studied  while  in  group  therapy  at 
a  state  hospital.  This  group  was  combined  with  a  sample  of  22  pa- 
tients, 4  male  and  18  female,  who  were  evaluated  in  an  outpatient 
clinic — diagnosed  as  psychotic  and  hospitalized.  A  third  sample  of 
20  patients  from  a  private  hospital  was  also  included.  The  total  psy- 
chotic sample  is,  therefore,  comprised  of  48  patients,  20  male  and  28 
female. 

This  sample  is  a  heterogeneous  mixture  of  cases.  No  claim  is  made 
that  they  are  representative  of  psychotics  in  general.  The  sample  is 
composed  of  at  least  two  different  types  of  psychotic  patients.  More 
than  half  are  paranoid,  i.e.,  underlying  sadism  or  distrust  covered  by 
a  fagade  of  pious  hypernormality.  The  other  group  includes  suicidal 
or  depressed  patients  who  have  a  double-  or  triple-level  structure  of 
despair,  resentment,  and/or  withdrawal.  The  statistics  of  the  Psy- 
chotic Sample  combine  the  results  from  both  these  dissimilar  groups. 
This  unquestionably  blurs  the  results. 

The  Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission  Sample 

A  group  of  207  patients,  73  male  and  134  female,  comprise  the 
Clinic  Admission  Sample.^  This  represents  all  the  patients  who  ap- 
plied for  diagnostic  evaluation  and  were  tested  in  the  Kaiser  Founda- 
tion Psychiatric  CUnic  over  a  six  months'  period.  This  sample  is  quite 
heterogeneous.  It  includes  some  severely  disturbed  patients,  some  self- 
referred  persons  seeking  psychotherapy;  but  the  largest  majority  of 
patients  in  the  clinic  admission  sample  were  referred  by  physicians  and 
came  under  the  pressure  of  somatic  or  psychosomatic  symptoms  or 
suffering  from  anxiety  which  was  not  internalized  or  attributed  to 

^  This  N  does  not  apply  to  Level  III.  At  the  "preconscious"  level  a  sample  of  100 
routine  clinic  patients  was  studied. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


379 


their  emorional  functioning.    These  patients  are  not  motivated  for 
therapy  and  this  is  reflected  in  their  test  responses. 

The  norms  for  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  system  are  based  on 
larger  samples  of  clinic  admissions.  Thus,  it  is  to  be  expected  that  the 
admission  samples  to  be  studied  in  this  chapter  will  fall  close  to  the 
center  of  the  diagnostic  circle  (i.e.,  the  mean).  These  selective  and 
normative  factors  are  important  in  considering  the  results  to  follow. 

Behavior  of  the  Ten  Samples  and  the  Three  Dermatitis 
Subsamples  at  Level  l-M 

The  average  scores  for  each  symptomatic  group  on  the  horizontal 
and  vertical  indices  were  obtained  and  plotted  on  a  master  diagnostic 
grid.  Figure  33  presents  the  mean  diagnostic  placement  for  each 
of  the  eight  samples  at  Level  I-M. 

This  diagram  indicates  that  five  of  the  groups  fall  in  the  extreme 
perimeter  of  the  upper  right-hand  quadrant,  thus  expressing  in  their 
symptomatic  behavior  strength  and  conventional  normality.  These  in- 
clude the  ulcer  (U),  hypertensive  (H),  obesity  (O),  the  unanxious 
dermatitis  (UD),  and  normal  control  (C)  samples.  The  self-inflicted 
dermatitis  sample  (SID)  falls  in  the  same  quadrant  but  expresses 
slightly  more  passivity  and  weakness. 

The  clinic  admission  sample  (A)  and  the  psychotic  sample  (P) 
fall  close  to  the  center  of  the  circle.  This  is  because  both  of  these 
groups  are  composed  of  two  types  of  people — those  who  are  denying 
symptoms  and  stressing  normality  and  those  who  are  admitting  weak- 
ness. The  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis  sample  (ND)  locates  in  the 
same  area. 

The  neurotic  group  (N)  manifests  an  extreme  amount  of  guilt  and 
passivity. 

Table  35  presents  the  statistical  tests  which  indicate  the  significance 
of  these  differences  among  the  symptomatic  groups. 

The  results  presented  in  Table  35  have  considerable  interest.  They 
indicate  that  the  neurotic  group  who  openly  accept  and  express  anx- 
iety are  significantly  different  (statistically)  from  every  other  group 
at  Level  I-M. 

No  distinction  can  be  made  at  this  level  between  the  ulcer,  hyper- 
tensive, obese,  unanxious  dermatitis,  self-inflicted  dermatitis,  and 
normal  control  samples.  This  means  that  these  six  groups  tend  to 
present  the  same  symptomless,  unanxious  fa9ade  and  cannot  be  dif- 
ferentially diagnosed  at  this  level. 

The  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis  sample  (ND)  is  significantly  more 
depressed  and  alienated  than  the  six  hypernormal  samples.  The  neu- 
rotic dermatitis  group  is  stronger  and  more  conventional  on  the  aver- 


38o 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


vS^^" 


!£!^[^S0CHIST1C 

^  (Hi) 


Figure  33.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  I-M. 


Code:  U  =  Ulcer 

H  =  Hypertensive 

O  =  Obese 

C  =  Dermatitis  Control 

A  =  Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission 


P  =  Psychotic 
N  =  Neurotic 
ND  =  Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis 
SID  =  Self-inflicted  Dermatitis 
UD  =  Unanxious  Dermatitis 


Key:  The  summary  placement  of  each  symptomatic  group  is  determined  by  the 
intersection  of  the  vertical  and  horizontal  indices.  The  indices  for  each  sample  were 
calculated  by  (1)  determining  the  number  of  cases  in  the  sample  falling  in  each  of  the 
eight  diagnostic  types  (at  Level  I-M)  and  (2)  feeding  these  numbers  into  the  formulas 
[Vertical  Index  =  l  —  5  +  .7(2  -(-  8  —  4  —  6)  and  Horizontal  Index  =  7  —  3  +  -7(6  + 
8  —  4  —  2),  where  1  =  the  number  of  subjects  falling  in  the  ^P-autocratic  sector  of 
the  circle  at  this  level  of  personality,  etc.]  The  resulting  indices  express  the  central 
trend  of  each  sample  in  comparison  with  the  other  seven  samples.  These  group 
indices  are  not  used  m  statistical  tests. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


38. 


TABLE  35 
The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups  at  Level  I-M 


SID   UD 


Number 

of 

Symptom 

Cases 

Group 

U 

H 

0 

c 

A 

p 

N 

ND 

39 

U 

_ 

41 

H 

NS 

_ 

100 

0 

NS 

NS 

- 

40 

C 

NS 

NS 

NS 

_ 

207 

A 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.001 

_ 

28 

P 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.001 

NS 

_ 

67 

N 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.20 

- 

31 

ND 

.01 

.01 

.001 

.01 

NS 

NS 

.02 

_ 

57 

SID 

NS 

NS 

NS 

NS 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.01 

73 

UD 

NS 

NS 

NS 

NS 

.001 

.001 

.001 

.001 

NS      - 

Key:  These  significance  tests  are  based  on  chi-squares— the  separation  being  be- 
tween the  number  of  patients  falling  (at  this  level)  in  the  strong  conventional  sectors 
(2,178)  and  the  number  of  patients  falling  in  the  weak-hostile  half  of  the  circle  (3,456). 
This  is  a  crude  over-aU  measure  which  fails  to  pick  up  specific  octant  differences. 
More  detailed  splits  (i.e.,  between  pairs  of  octants)  would  increase  the  significance 
indices. 

age  than  the  neurotic  group.  It  does  not  differ  from  the  psychotic  and 
clinic  admission  samples. 

These  similarities  and  differences  allow  us  to  combine  the  groups 
(at  Level  I-M)  into  four  categories: 

,^  J  ,  ,  f  Ulcer,  hypersensitive,  obesity,  nor- 

Very  strong  and  hypernormal  |^^j  ^^J/^^^  unanxious  dermatitis 

Fairly  strong  Self-inflicted  dermatitis 

Fairly  weak,  somewhat  f  Psychiatric  admissions,  psychotics, 

depressed  and  dependent  Ineurotic  dermatitis 

Very  weak  and  dependent  Patients  in  psychotherapy 

Behavior  of  the  Ten  Samples  at  Level  II-C 

The  mean  Level  II-C  indices  of  the  ten  symptomatic  groups  were 
plotted  on  the  diagnostic  grid.  These  results  are  diagramed  in  Figure 
34. 

We  observe  that  these  results  are  somewhat  different  from  the 
Level  I  scores.  The  neurotic  sample  (N)  again  falls  in  the  passive, 
weak  sector  of  the  diagnostic  circle.  The  chnic  admission  sample  (A) 
and  the  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis  sample  (ND)  again  fall  near  the 
center.  The  self-inflicted  dermatitis  sample  again  falls  in  the  upper 
right-hand  quadrant  indicating  a  moderate  claiming  of  strong,  re- 
sponsible behavior. 


382         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


V^^N^ 


.^^^" 


^c,^\C 


MANAGERMt- 


X4p; 


.N^ 


xAV 


•u 


^^V 


%: 


UO/ 


\ 


/ 

0 V.      . 

■  ^  >4sio 

^  ,  .  l?l  1  .  . 

,  ■  'r  M , 

r\ 

1  1  1  1  M  1  .  1 

_ 

;\     -^^^ 

l^ir. 


^rn 


•N 


# 


^2^S0CHIST\C 


FiGxniE  34.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  II-C.    Code  and  Key:  Same  as  for 
Figure  33. 

The  normal  control  group  (C)  shifts.  At  Level  I  they  manifest 
the  strongest  and  most  symptomless  fa9ade.  In  their  conscious  self- 
descriptions,  they  do  not  emphasize  strength,  they  admit  to  some 
hostile  and  weak  behavior  and  almost  duplicate  the  sm  sample. 

The  ulcer  group  (U)  also  shifts.  In  their  symptomatic  presenta- 
tion (Level  I-M),  they  stress  strength  and  conventionality.  They 
consciously  claim  to  be  tougher  and  more  aggressive.  They  are  (at 
Level  II-C)  the  most  independent  and  hardboiled  of  any  symptom 
group. 

The  hypertensives  (H)  stay  (at  Level  II-C)  in  the  responsible, 
hypernormal  octant,  although  there  is  a  greater  emphasis  on  their 
strength. 

The  obesity  group  (O)  remains  power-oriented  and  produces  a 
double-level  fa9ade  of  executive  strength.   The  unanxious  dermatitis 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  383 

(UD)  is  the  third  most  hypernormal  managerial  group  at  the  level  of 
self-diagnosis. 

The  psychotic  group  (P)  shows  a  decided  shift.  At  Level  I-M  they 
are  scattered  between  passivity  and  hypernormality.  At  the  level  of 
conscious  self-description  they  become  overwhelmingly  hypernormal. 
This  severely  disturbed  group  thus  diagnoses  itself  as  responsible  and 
executive! 

Table  36  presents  the  statistical  tests  which  indicate  the  significance 
of  these  differences.    These  results  are  worth  comment.   The  ulcer 

TABLE  36 

The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups  at  the 
Level  of  Conscious  Self-Description  (Level  II-C) 


SID   UD 


of 

Symptom 

Cases 

Group 

U 

H 

0 

c 

A 

p 

N 

ND 

42 

u 

_ 

49 

H 

.01 

_ 

100 

0 

.001 

NS 

_ 

41 

C 

NS 

.01 

.001 

_ 

207 

A 

.20 

.001 

.001 

.20 

_ 

26 

P 

NS 

.01 

.001 

NS 

NS 

_ 

67 

N 

.02 

.001 

.001 

.02 

.10 

.20 

_ 

31 

ND 

NS 

.001 

.001 

NS 

NS 

NS 

.10 

_ 

56 

SID 

NS 

.001 

.001 

NS 

NS 

NS 

.05 

NS 

70 

UD 

NS 

.05 

.01 

NS 

.001 

.10 

.001 

.10 

.05 

Key:  These  significance  tests  are  based  on  chi-squares— the  separation  being  be- 
tween the  number  of  patients  falling  (at  this  level)  in  the  strong  conventional  sectors 
(2,178)  and  the  number  of  patients  failing  in  the  weak-hostile  half  of  the  circle  (3,456). 
This  is  a  crude  over-all  measure  which  fails  to  pick  up  specific  octant  differences. 
More  detailed  splits  (i.e.,  between  pairs  of  octants)  would  increase  the  significance 
indices. 

sample  is  extremely  different  from  the  obese  and  hypertensive  samples. 
The  latter  claim  more  conventional  hypernormal  traits.  The  ulcer 
sample  is  also  different  from  the  neurotics  who  admit  to  considerably 
more  passivity. 

The  obesity  and  hypertensive  groups  are  very  similar  to  each 
other  and  are  significantly  different  from  every  other  sample.  No 
other  group  approaches  their  strength. 

The  neurotic  sample  is  unique — no  other  group  approaches  the 
weakness  they  manifest. 

The  panic-psychotic  sample  is  significantly  more  "normal"  in  its 
self-diagnosis  than  the  neurotics — and  shows  no  difference  from  the 
normal  dermatitis  and  ulcer  samples. 

The  similarities  and  differences  allow  us  to  combine  the  groups 
at  Level  II-C  into  the  following  general  categories: 


384        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

,.  ,  ,  ,  /Hypertensive,  obesity,  unanxious 

Very  strong  and  hypernormal  ijer^atitis 

, ,  J         ,  J  ,  ,  /Normals,  psychotics,  self-inflicted 

Moderately  strong  and  hypernormal  i,  •  •  ^  ^ 

Very  strong  and  aggressive  Ulcer 

Very  weak  Neurotic 

No  commitment  to  any  modal  fClinic  admission,  overtly  neurotic 

security  operation  Idermatitis 

Behavior  of  the  Ten  Samples  at  Level  III-T 

The  average  scores  (at  Level  III-T  Hero)  for  each  sample  were 
plotted  on  the  diagnostic  grid.    Figure  35  indicates  that  dramatic 


^^"- 

i^"- 


:!^[^S0CHIST1C 
^  (HI) 


Figure  35.  Behavior  of  Ten  Samples  at  Level  III-T  (Hero).  Code  and  Key:  Same  as 
for  Figure  33. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  385 

shifts  in  behavior  occur  when  this  underlying  level  is  brought  into 
play. 

The  sample,  which  at  the  underlying  level  manifests  the  most 
strength,  is  the  normal  control  (C).  They  fall  in  the  competitive, 
narcissistic  sector  of  the  circle.  The  obesity  sample  (O)  also  locates 
in  this  sector,  but  they  are  not  as  power-oriented  as  the  controls.  At 
the  level  of  conscious  self-description,  it  will  be  recalled  that  the 
obese  sample  claimed  to  be  much  stronger  than  the  controls.  While 
they  do  not  maintain  this  dominance  in  relation  to  the  controls,  they 
remain  in  "preconscious"  behavior  the  second  most  confident  and 
independent  of  all  the  samples. 

The  unanxious  dermatitis  sample  (UD)  expresses  underlying 
themes  of  strength  and  hostility.  This  group  is  significantly  weaker 
than  the  controls  and  significantly  more  hostile  than  psychiatric 
clinic  (A)  sample.  The  self-inflicted  dermatitis  sample  expresses  more 
underlying  sadism  than  any  other  group. 

The  hypertensive  (H)  sample  is  the  next  most  hostile.  This  symp- 
tom group  is  more  committed  to  underlying  sadistic  feelings  than  the 
neurotic  and  clinic  samples. 

The  ulcer  (U)  sample  clearly  differs  significantly  from  all  other 
psychosomatic  groups  (at  Level  III-T).  This  group  expresses  more 
passivity  and  more  positive  trustful  themes  than  any  other  psychoso- 
matic group.  The  ulcer  sample  is  considerably  more  passive  than  the 
neurotic  group,  for  example.  The  psychotic  group  (P)  presents  more 
underlying  submissiveness  and  weakness  than  any  other  sample. 

TABLE  37 

The  Significance  of  Differences  Among  Ten  Symptomatic  Groups  at  the 
Level  of  "Preconscious"  Expression  (Level  III-T  [Hero]) 


SID   UD 


of 

Symptom 

Cases 

Group 

U 

H 

0 

C 

A 

P 

N 

ND 

19 

U 

_ 

27 

H 

NS 

_ 

97 

0 

.05 

.10 

- 

41 

C 

.001 

.001 

.05 

_ 

103 

A 

NS 

NS 

.05 

.001 

_ 

18 

P 

NS 

NS 

.05 

.001 

NS 

_ 

67 

N 

.20 

NS 

NS 

.01 

NS 

.20 

_ 

33 

ND 

.20 

NS 

NS 

.01 

NS 

.20 

NS 

_ 

57 

SID 

NS 

NS 

.10 

.001 

NS 

NS 

NS 

NS 

71 

UD 

.02 

.05 

NS 

.05 

.02 

.05 

NS 

NS 

.05 

Key:  These  significance  tests  are  based  on  chi-squares— the  separation  being  be- 
tween the  number  of  patients  falling  (at  this  level)  in  the  strong  conventional  sectors 
(2,178)  and  the  number  of  patients  falling  in  the  weak-hostile  half  of  the  circle  (3,456). 
This  is  a  crude  over-all  measure  which  fails  to  pick  up  specific  octant  differences. 
More  detailed  splits  (i.e.,  between  pairs  of  octants)  would  increase  the  significance 
indices. 


386         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

Table  37  presents  the  significance  tests  for  the  differences  among 
these  ten  samples.  The  most  interesting  aspect  of  this  table  is  the 
large  number  of  significant  differences  among  the  psychosomatic  and 
normal  control  groups.  The  Level  III-T  (Hero)  score  is  clearly  the 
most  sensitive  and  powerful  instrument  for  discriminating  among 
these  symptom  groups.  The  normal  control  group,  for  example,  is 
significantly  different  from  every  other  sample — neurotic,  psychotic, 
and  psychosomatic. 

These  results  lead  us  to  conclude  that  several  psychosomatic  groups 
cannot  be  differentiated  from  each  other  or  from  normal,  psychotic, 
or  psychiatric  samples  at  the  two  overt  levels,  but  they  can  be  sepa- 
rated with  statistical  significance  by  the  use  of  a  Level  III  measuring 
device.  The  importance  of  a  multilevel  approach  is  underlined  by 
these  findings. 

The  "preconscious"  expressions  of  the  ten  samples  can  be  sum- 
marized as  follows: 

,  r  11         1  rNormal,  obesity,  unanxious 

Very  strong  and  independent  jdermatitis 

Mildly  independent  Neurotic 

T^  ,         ...  rHvpertensive,  self-inflicted 

Extremely  sadistic  id         rr' 

Most  dependent  and  masochistic  Ulcer  and  psychotic 

No  trend  (normative  group)  Clinic  admission 

In  the  last  few  pages  we  have  studied  three  levels  of  personality, 
discussing,  in  turn,  the  behavior  of  the  ten  samples  at  each  level.  We 
shall  now  focus,  not  on  the  levels,  but  on  the  symptomatic  groups, 
pulling  together  the  multilevel  pattern  for  each  sample. 

Multilevel  Personality  Pattern  of  Norwal  Controls 

When  the  average  scores  of  the  normal  group  at  Level  I-M,  Level 
II-C,  and  Level  III-T  (Hero)  are  plotted  on  the  same  diagnostic  grid, 
we  obtain  the  multilevel  picture  for  this  sample.  Figure  36  presents 
these  plottings.  At  the  symptomatic  level,  the  normals  are,  as  expected, 
strong  and  generate  no  dependent  or  helpless  pressure.  In  their  con- 
scious self-descriptions,  they  are  much  less  oriented  towards  power. 
Whereas  they  all  have  the  same  symptom-free  fa9ade  at  Level  I, 
they  are  quite  heterogeneous  at  Level  II-C.  This  also  seems  to  make 
sense.  It  indicates  that  normals  differ  in  their  perception  of  their  own 
interpersonal  roles.  Some  claim  independence,  some  docile  conform- 
ity, some  responsibility,  etc. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


387 


managerml- 


3' 


^ 


^-^ 


/  i? 
Q 


'^V 


•I 


/•n 


"^s: 


/ 

V^/-^' 

V 

"^z 

^^__Jr 

"^ , , ,  1 , , , ,',°, , , , 

/\ 

X 

/ 

\ 

^^^r^^  \ 

\    / 

A 

\ 

N    ' 

\  / 

■  \ 

-^ 

\ 

'^/^ 


/^^/ 


/^^><, 


£!^(^SOCHISTlC 


# 
^^'^i^ 


Figure  36.  The  Mean  Scores  of  38  Normal  Controls  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T 
(Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices  em- 
ployed in  Figures  33,  34,  and  35. 

In  their  underlying  feelings,  they  return  to  their  Level  I  strength 
and  independence.  Their  "preconscious"  themes  are  more  self-confi- 
dent and  narcissistic  than  any  other  sample. 

The  total  pattern  stresses  executive,  autonomous  operations.  No 
typical  conflict  exists  for  this  group.  A  triple  level  solidity  and  com- 
petence is  suggested. 

Clinical  Implications 

The  multilevel  pattern  we  have  just  sketched  represents  the  group 
tendency  of  a  sample  of  normal  subjects.  Needless  to  say,  the  indi- 
vidual patients  in  the  normal  sample  do  not  all  manifest  this  personal- 
ity pattern.  No  generalizations  about  this  pattern  hold  for  individual 
subjects.  It  may  be  suggestive,  however,  to  point  out  the  clinical  im- 


388        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

plication  of  this  generic  pattern — which  would  hold  for  many  normal 
subjects. 

Patients  with  this  record  would  be  considered  as  unmotivated  for 
psychotherapy.  The  strong,  symptom-free  Level  I-M,  the  moderately 
conventional  self-perception,  and  the  self-confident,  independent, 
"preconscious"  feelings  all  point  to  a  lack  of  desire  for  help  or  per- 
sonality change. 

The  three  layers  of  strength  suggest  a  durable  character  structure 
with  little  interlevel  conflict.  The  prognosis  points  to  little  change. 
They  will  tend  to  maintain  their  strong  security  operations. 

Multilevel  Personality  Pattern  of  Ulcer  Patients 

The  average  scores  at  three  levels  for  the  ulcer  sample  are  plotted 
in  Figure  37.   At  the  symptomatic  level,  the  ulcer  group  stresses  re- 


^^^f 


HANAGERUi 


•TL 


mocfi^ 


Ar,^ 


Mf 


^^F, 


\ 


:!£(^^S0CHIST1C 


Figure  37.  The  Mean  Scores  of  41  Ulcer  Patients  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T 
(Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices  em- 
ployed in  Figures  33,  34,  and  35, 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  389 

sponsibility  and  conventional  strength.  They  do  not  admit  to  emo- 
tional symptoms.  They  manifest  bland,  hypernormal  behavior,  more 
so  than  do  the  normals. 

In  their  conscious  self-descriptions,  they  emphasize  tough,  inde- 
pendent self-confidence;  they  claim  to  be  more  aggressive  than  any 
other  group. 

In  their  "preconscious"  imagery,  a  marked  conflict  appears.  They 
express  passivity  and  dependence. 

The  total  personality  pattern  reveals  a  tremendous  interlevel 
variability.  This  conflict  between  overt  responsible  independence 
and  covert  passivity  is  in  general  agreement  w^ith  the  findings  of  other 
research  workers  who  have  studied  the  ulcer  personality.  Internists, 
analytic,  and  nonpsychoanalytic  writers  alike  (1,  2,  3,  4)  have  stressed 
the  energetic,  success-oriented  traits  overtly  claimed  by  ulcer  pa- 
tients. Psychoanalytic  authorities  have  further  indicated  that  passivity 
and  dependence  underlie  this  strong  fa9ade.  (1,  2)  Our  results  offer 
objective  evidence  in  support  of  these  earlier  clinical  studies. 

Clinical  Implications 

Patients  with  this  profile  would  present  a  tricky  diagnostic  prob- 
lem, and  motivation  for  therapy  is  mixed.  They  present  a  responsible, 
aggressive,  independent  fagade.  This  generally  indicates  a  low  moti- 
vation for  treatment.  These  security  operations  accompany  a  denial 
of  the  need  for  help. 

The  underlying  hostile  passivity  tends  to  work  in  the  opposite 
direction.  Strong  "preconscious"  dependence  is  suggested.  This  is 
often  manifested  indirectly.  These  patients  are  often  too  proud  to  ask 
for  help,  but  their  underlying  feelings  of  helplessness  may  appear 
obliquely.  A  nurturant,  poised  behavior  on  the  part  of  the  therapist 
may  link  up  with  the  patient's  covert  passivity. 

The  prognosis  for  patients  with  this  extreme  conflict  is  also  mixed. 
These  patients  will  inevitably  become  "sicker"  during  therapy.  The 
sturdy,  symptom-free  facade  wilts,  and  depressive,  fearful,  depend- 
ent feelings  develop. 

The  prognostic  issue  is  this:  Can  a  patient  with  this  severe  conflict 
tolerate  the  emergence  of  the  underlying  feelings  of  helplessness? 
Two  layers  of  overt  operations  are  committed  to  maintaining  strength. 
In  some  cases,  the  pressure  of  treatment  (which  inevitably  involves 
tapping  the  "preconscious  dependence")  may  cause  unbearable 
anxiety.  This  may  lead  to  a  "flight-into-health"  or  depressive  episodes, 
or  long-term  obsessive  solutions. 

The  clinical  course  of  a  patient  with  a  conflict  between  overt 
strength  and  underlying  weakness  is  always  complicated  and  un- 


390 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


certain.  The  task  of  the  therapist  is  to  be  sensitive  to  the  patient's  re- 
action to  emerging  passivity  and  to  the  signs  of  intensified  anxiety. 

Multilevel  Personality  Pattern  of  Hypertensive  Patients 

The  average  indices  at  three  levels  for  the  hypertensive  sample  is 
plotted  in  Figure  38.  At  the  symptomatic  level,  the  hypertensive 
group  falls  in  the  responsible-generous  sector  of  the  circle — close  to 
the  ulcer  sample.  In  conscious  self-description,  they  emphasize  power 


^;^< 


l*^NAG£fiM^ 


U^) 


^  (HI) 


Figure  38.  The  Mean  Scores  of  49  Hypertensive  Patients  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and 
III-T  (Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices 
employed  in  Figures  33,  34,  and  35. 


and  independence.    On  the  "preconscious"  measures  they  manifest 
more  sadism  than  any  other  symptomatic  group. 

A  severe  conflict  is  indicated.    The  fa9ade  of  conventional  re- 
sponsible strength  covers  intense  feelings  of  rage. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  391 

These  findings  tend  to  support  the  chnical  folklore  about  hyper- 
tensive patients,  which  describes  them  as  denying  hostility.  The 
psychosomatic  literature  is  less  specific  in  its  discussions  of  hyper- 
tensive patients.  Saul  points  out  that  "on  the  surface  these  indi- 
viduals were  non-hostile  and  even  overly  gentle,  but  did  not  lack 
energy.  They  worked — in  fact  overworked — and  succeeded,  while 
protesting  against  doing  so"  (5,  p.  159).  Our  objective  data  support 
this  statement. 

Clinical  Implications 

It  is  not  possible,  on  the  basis  of  these  data,  to  generalize  about  all 
hypertensive  patients.  We  can,  however,  suggest  the  clinical  impli- 
cations for  an  individual  who  manifests  this  particular  multilevel  pat- 
tern. 

This  profile  does  not  indicate  motivation  for  therapy.  The  self- 
satisfied,  responsible  fa9ade  does  not  lead  the  person  to  seek  or  accept 
the  role  of  a  patient.  In  their  overt  operations  they  are,  thus,  similar 
to  the  ulcer  sample. 

The  underlying  material  is  also  mobilized  against  psychotherapy. 
The  underlying  hostility  could  be  expected  to  be  a  barrier  between 
the  patient  and  the  therapist.  The  ulcer  patient's  covert  passivity 
functions  to  pull  the  patient  (against  his  conscious  desire)  into  a  de- 
pendent relationship.  The  hypertensive  patient  is  pushed  (against  his 
conscious  desire)  in  the  direction  of  angry  irritation. 

Patients  with  this  pattern  have  a  poor  prognosis  for  psycho- 
therapy. They  overtly  say  that  they  do  not  need  help  and  they 
covertly  offer  a  thorny,  violent  picture. 

These  patients  are  made  very  anxious  by  the  prospect  of  treatment. 
They  generally  try  to  "repress"  out  of  therapy  and  to  deny  any 
unconventional  feelings.  If  they  do  get  involved  in  treatment,  they 
often  become  paralyzed  with  anxiety  and/or  suffused  with  righteous 
anger  against  the  therapist. 

In  the  Kaiser  Foundation  Clinic,  we  have  found  hypertensives  to 
be  among  the  most  difficult  candidates  for  psychotherapy.  They  are 
considered  to  have  a  poor  prognosis  for  therapy. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  Obesity  Sample 

The  generalizations  made  in  this  section  about  the  obesity  group  are 
even  more  tentative  than  those  made  about  the  other  samples — first, 
because  of  different  selection  procedures  and  secondly,  because  only 
female  subjects  are  involved. 

The  average  scores  at  three  levels  for  the  obesity  sample  are  plotted 
in  Figure  39. 


392 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


•HT 


MANAGERMl- 


I4P; 


q/ 


moc, 


''^>. 


s<9 


&L^. 


f^r. 


o^"- 

^^. 


,^^' 

^^e^^' 


Figure  39.  The  Mean  Scores  of  98  Obese  Females  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T 
(Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  mdices  em- 
ployed in  Figiires  33,  34,  and  35. 

Power  and  narcissistic  pride  are  emphasized  at  all  levels.  This  sam- 
ple is,  along  with  the  normals,  the  least  conflicted.  A  rigid  clinging 
to  the  same  security  operations  at  each  level  is  indicated. 

Clinical  Implications 

These  findings  suggest  that  obese  women  are  extremely  power- 
oriented.  They  tend  to  duplicate  the  pattern  of  the  normal  controls 
except  that  they  are  significantly  stronger  than  the  normals  at  Level 
II.  They  claim  inordinate  strength. 

There  are  several  theoretical  implications.  The  need  to  be  "big," 
to  occupy  space,  to  swing  one's  weight  may  be  motivated  by  and 
correlated  with  this  personality  pattern.  The  refusal  of  many  obese 
patients  to  accept  and  obey  dietary  regimes  may  be  tied  to  the  stub- 
born, narcissistic  security  operations  which  our  instruments  measure. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES  393 

To  the  extent  that  this  triple-level  pattern  holds  for  other  obese 
groups,  it  seems  clear  that  any  therapeutic  approach  (medical  or 
psychological)  is  doomed  to  failure  if  it  does  not  take  into  account 
these  power  strivings. 

These  patients  are  not  motivated  for  psychotherapy.  They  are 
free  from  the  classic  emotional  symptoms.  They  are  not  at  all  self- 
critical  or  dependent.  Their  independence  and  autonomy  would  be 
threatened  by  the  prospect  of  treatment.  Giving  up  their  weight  or 
their  narcissistic  strength  would  obviously  run  counter  to  their  triple- 
level  security  operations  and  would  be  attended  with  considerable 
anxiety. 

Patients  with  this  personality  pattern  rarely  enter  treatment.  If 
they  do,  an  intense  power  struggle  with  the  therapist  usually  results. 

A  three  layer  commitment  to  the  same  security  operation  indicates 
a  rigid  personality — with  little  change  expected. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Sample 

The  mean  indices  at  three  levels  of  the  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis 
sample  are  profiled  in  Figure  40.  At  the  symptomatic  level,  this  sam- 
ple locates  in  the  same  sector  of  the  diagnostic  grid  as  the  psychiatric 
clinic  admission  group.  They  are  not  as  strong  as  the  psychosomatic 
groups  and  not  as  weak  as  the  neurotic  sample. 

At  the  level  of  conscious  self-description,  the  neurotic  dermatitis 
group  again  falls  close  to  the  psychiatric  clinic  sample.  They  are  mid- 
way between  the  psychosomatics  and  neurotic  samples. 

In  their  "preconscious"  expressions,  the  neurotic  dermatitis  sam- 
ple is  mildly  aggressive. 

This  sample  manifests  a  multilevel  personality  pattern  which  is 
very  close  at  all  three  levels  to  the  psychiatric  clinic  admission  sample. 
They  are  also  much  closer  to  the  psychotic  and  neurotic  samples  (at 
the  fa9ade  levels,  I  and  II)  than  the  other  psychosomatic  groups. 
These  findings  make  it  seem  reasonable  to  label  the  acne,  psoriasis, 
and  seborrheic  dermatitis  samples  as  the  overtly  neurotic  dermatitis 
subcluster. 

Clinical  Implications 

The  neurotic  dermatitis  group  manifests  more  depression,  guilt,  and 
alienation  than  any  other  psychosomatic  sample.  It  might  be  as- 
sumed that  they  recognize  their  problems  and  anxieties  whereas  the 
other  psychosomatic  groups  clearly  deny  these  emotions.  For  this 
reason  it  seems  that  acne,  psoriasis,  and  seborrheic  patients  would  be 
more  likely  to  accept  psychotherapy.  They  would  tend  to  be  more 


394 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


^^f'' 


57/77T 

Figure  40.  The  Mean  Scores  of  3 1  Overtly  Neurotic  Dermatitis  Patients  at  Levels 
I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T  (Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based 
on  the  indices  employed  m  Figures  33,  34,  and  35. 

morivated  for  self-exploration.    They  are  clearly  less  self-satisfied 
and  bland  in  their  overt  operations. 

This  is  not  to  say  that  all  acne  patients,  for  example,  are  depressed 
and  eager  for  treatment.  A  higher  percentage  is  likely  to  be  found  in 
this  group  than  in  the  other  psychosomatic  samples. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Sample 

The  average  scores  of  the  self-inflicted  dermatitis  sample  are  pre- 
sented in  Figure  41.  These  patients  are  hypernormal  at  the  fagade 
levels  (I  and  II).  Although  they  do  not  manifest  as  much  conven- 
tional strength  as  the  controls  or  the  other  psychosomatics  they  are 
not  significantly  different  from  these  hypernormal  samples. 

At  Level  III-T  these  patients  present  more  sadism  than  any  other 
sample.  The  multilevel  pattern  is  that  of  a  sweet,  responsible  fa9ade 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


395 


MANAGffiMi-. 


*^\^ 


^-^" 


N^^' 


Uj 


-moco 


'ATic 


•  I 


%: 


% 


\          •™-             ^^N^^     \ 

li__— -—-^ 

^V^/. 


"^/■z 


L^^^A 


£^^[^sochist>c 
^7h7) " 


4^ 


Figure  41.  The  Mean  Scores  of  57  Self-inflicted  Dermatitis  Patients  at  Levels  I-M, 
II-C,  and  III-T  (Hero). 

with  underlying  rage  and  bitterness.  In  light  of  the  self-destructive 
nature  of  their  self-inflicted  (or  self -exacerbated)  symptoms  the  find- 
ing of  intense  underlying  hostility  is  of  some  interest. 

Clinical  Implications 

There  are  definite  clinical  correlates  of  the  modal  personality  type 
exhibited  by  the  group  of  dermatitis  patients  which  comprise  the  ex- 
coriation sample.  In  the  first  place  the  fa9ade  of  righteous  responsibil- 
ity suggests  that  they  would  not  be  well  motivated  for  psychotherapy. 
They  tend  to  present  themselves  as  conventional  people  and  to  mani- 
fest the  emotional  symptoms  of  the  hysterical  or  hypemormal  per- 
sonality types.  They  might  be  quite  threatened  by  the  attempt  to 
steer  them  towards  a  psychiatric  clinic.  The  typical  case  would  ex- 
hibit an  intense  conflict  between  this  bland  exterior  and  underlying 


396        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

sadistic  rage.  The  hostility  is  apparently  expressed  indirectly  through 
self -mutilation.  Self-exploration  would  tend  to  disturb  their  equilib- 
rium and  might  produce  a  severe  anxiety  reaction.  This  might  be  re- 
solved by  an  increase  in  the  fagade  operations  (i.e.,  further  intensifi- 
cation of  the  hypernormal  response)  or  by  a  break  through  of  the 
internal  anger  which,  at  the  minimum,  would  rupture  the  doctor- 
patient  relationship  and,  at  the  worst,  result  in  psychotic  aggression. 
The  following  clinical  implication  suggests  itself:  patients  with 
the  dermatological  symptoms  of  atopic  dermatitis,  otitis  externa, 
pruritis,  neurotic  excoriations  should  not  be  rushed  into  a  psychiatric 
referral.  Any  exploration  of  emotional  factors  should  proceed  with 
cautious  tentativeness  and  avoid  crashing  headlong  into  a  strongly  de- 
fended, bland,  self-satisfied  fagade.  These  patients  may  appear  to  be 
nervous.  They  may  admit  to  "tension"  or  to  being  "high-strung,"  but 
they  still  tend  to  cling  to  a  repressive,  hypernormal  self-image 
and  to  resist  psychotherapeutic  procedures.  In  some  cases  it  might  be 
expected  that  psychotic  episodes  would  follow  a  breakdown  in  the 
precarious  defenses. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Unanxious  Dermatitis  Sample 

The  modal  scores  of  the  unanxious  dermatitis  sample  are  profiled 
in  Figure  42.  This  group  is,  at  all  levels,  stronger  and  more  self- 
confident  than  the  other  dermatitis  groups.  They  tend  to  be  much 
more  like  the  normal  controls.  They  express  much  less  anxiety  and 
self-effacement  than  the  other  dermatitis  groups. 

The  fact  that  this  group  falls  so  close  to  the  controls  tends  to  throw 
doubt  on  the  presence  of  emotional  factors.  These  data  suggest  that 
hyperhydrotic  eczema,  alopecia  areata,  urticaria,  acne  rosacea,  lupus 
erythematous,  herpes  simplex,  and  warts  are  not  psychosomatic  dis- 
orders and  that  organic,  physiological  factors  may  play  a  more  de- 
cisive role  in  the  development  of  these  symptoms. 

The  unanxious  subgroup  is  significantly  different  from  the  neurotic 
dermatitis  and  the  self-inflicted  dermatitis  samples.  The  evidence  pre- 
sented in  this  chapter  suggests  the  hypothesis  that  the  symptomatic 
groups  which  comprise  the  neurotic  and  self-inflicted  dermatitis  sam- 
ples are  definitely  psychosomatic  while  the  unanxious  sample  is  not. 

Clinical  Implications 

The  multilevel  picture  in  the  case  of  the  unanxious  dermatitis  sam- 
ple involves  three  layers  of  strength  and  narcissistic  self-confidence. 
The  clinical  implications  are  obvious.  These  patients  are  not  emo- 
tionally upset.    They  are  not  motivated  for  psychotherapy.    They 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


397 


'IE 


>C    V 

/^ 

^_-^ 

^^'^1 

0            .              20 

JO 

Jo                 \sA 

'0 

90                            9 

^^ 

^' 

^Vt^. 


/^^A 


?^4C. 


-J 


'*(?- MASOCHISTIC 


^^^:i%t;^ 


Figure  42.    The  Mean  Scores  of  73  Unanxious  Dermatitis  Patients  at  Levels  I-M, 
II-C,  and  III-T  (Hero). 

will  tend  to  maintain  self-confident,  independent  operations  and  to 
resist  psychotherapy  or  a  psychiatric  referral. 

There  does  not  seem  to  be  as  much  danger  of  provoking  anxiety  in 
these  patients.  The  self-inflicted  dermatitis  cases  might  be  threatened 
by  the  prospect  of  self-exploration.  The  average  unanxious  derma- 
titis patient  would  not  be  disturbed  by  a  referral  to  psychotherapy — 
he  probably  would  actively  and  independently  refuse  it. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Psychiatric  Clinic  Admission  Sample 

The  average  scores  of  the  clinic  admission  sample  at  three  levels 
are  diagramed  in  Figure  43.  This  sample  is  composed  of  all  patients 
tested  during  a  six-month  period  at  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychiatric 
Clinic.   A  very  heterogeneous  group  of  patients  is  included  in  this 


398 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


MANAGERIAL 


^!£!^!£;^SOCHlST\C 

^(Hl) 

Figure  43.  The  Mean  Scores  of  207  Psychiatric  Chnic  Admission  Patients  at  Levels 
I-M  and  II-C,  and  100  Clinic  Admission  Patients  at  Level  III-T  (Hero).  Key:  The 
summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices  employed  in  Figures  33, 
34,  and  35. 

sample  (i.e.,  severe  neurotics,  psychosomatics,  patients  wanting  ther- 
apy, and  patients  strongly  mobilized  against  treatment).  For  this 
reason  we  should  not  expect  a  definite  trend  towards  any  particular 
interpersonal  operations. 

The  norms  used  for  diagnosis  are  based  on  approximately  800  clinic 
admissions.  The  present  group  of  207  was  included  in  the  normative 
sample. 

This  is  a  second  and  more  convincing  reason  to  expect  that  the 
means  of  the  admission  sample  will  fall  close  to  the  center  of  the  diag- 
nostic grid. 

The  plottings  presented  in  Figure  43  therefore,  have  little  diag- 
nostic meaning  but  they  do  sen^e  as  a  reference  point  to  which  the 
other  samples  can  be  related. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


399 


Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Neurotic  Sample 

Figure  44  presents  the  three-level  mean  scores  for  the  neurotic 
(psychotherapy)  sample.  This  group  comprises  patients  who  were 
seen  in  group  or  individual  psychotherapy.  This  group  is,  by  defini- 
tion, heterogeneous,  since  patients  M'ith  several  psychiatric  diagnoses 
are  assigned  to  therapy.  The  majority  of  these  patients  fall  in  the 
schizoid,  obsessive,  phobic,  and  hysteric  categories.  The  mean  scores 
at  each  level  are,  therefore,  the  resultant  of  different  interpersonal 
pressures.  The  generalizations  to  follow  are  limited  by  this  qualifica- 
tion. 

At  Level  I  the  therapy  sample  averages  out  to  be  submissive,  pas- 
sive, dependent.  Some  of  the  neurotics  were  bitter  and  some  conven- 


:i^[^SOCHlSTlC 
^  (HI) 

Figure  44.  The  Mean  Scores  of  67  "Neurotic"  Patients  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T 
(Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices  em- 
ployed in  Figures  33,  34,  and  35. 


400        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

tionally  agreeable  so  that  the  horizontal  (love-hate)  factor  balances 
out.  The  large  majority  of  the  neurotics  were  depressed,  anxious,  and 
fearful  which  results  in  a  Level  I-M  score  which  is  considerably 
weaker  than  any  other  sample. 

At  Level  II-C  they  see  themselves  in  the  same  way. 

In  their  "preconscious"  expressions,  a  drastic  change  occurs.  Feel- 
ings of  narcissism  and  independence  appear.  At  the  facade  levels,  the 
neurotics  were,  by  far,  the  weakest  group.  They  use  passivity  as  their 
security  operation  at  these  levels.  In  their  fantasies  they  are  stronger 
than  four  other  samples.  The  psychotherapy  patients  tend  to  use 
overt  weakness  to  cover  underlying  feelings  of  narcissism  and  self- 
enhancement.  They  are,  at  the  preconscious  level,  not  as  docile,  de- 
pendent, and  timid  as  they  claim  to  be  or  as  they  overtly  appear  to  be. 

Clinical  Implications 

This  sample  is  so  diverse  that  specific  generalizations  are  limited. 
We  can  say  that  the  neurotic  sample  is  initially  well  motivated  for 
therapy  in  that  they  present  an  anxious,  worried,  dependent  fagade. 
They  are  consciously  dissatisfied  with  themselves  and  eager  for  help. 

The  underlying  scores  tell  us  that  a  typical  conflict  exists  and  that 
they  are  not  as  weak  as  they  claim  to  be.  The  "preconscious"  narcis- 
sism might  predict,  in  some  cases,  to  power  struggles  and  feelings  of 
superiority  which  may  be  used  against  the  therapist  or  which  may 
point  to  potential  self-confidence  and  self-acceptance. 

Multilevel  Pattern  of  the  Psychotic  Sample 

The  average  indices  of  the  hospitalized  psychotic  sample  at  Levels 
I,  II,  and  III  are  presented  in  Figure  45.  In  their  symptomatic  be- 
havior, this  group  falls  close  to  the  clinic  admission  sample.  This  is 
because  of  the  diversity  within  the  sample.  Some  psychotics  manifest 
symptoms  of  passivity  and  depression;  but  more  of  them,  however, 
tend  to  deny  symptoms  and  present  as  strong,  healthy  persons.  The 
mean  falls  close  to  the  center.  It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the  psy- 
chotics are  considerably  stronger  and  more  self-possessed  (at  Level 
I-M)  than  the  neurotics  who  are  willing  to  internalize  and  admit  to 
symptoms. 

In  their  conscious  self-descriptions,  the  psychotic  group  takes  a 
unique  position.  In  their  own  perceptions  they  are  conventionally 
normal!   They  claim  pious  sweetness  and  cooperative  sociability. 

The  results  of  the  underlying  tests  tend  to  shatter  this  two-layer 
fa9ade.  Intense  feelings  of  deprivation  and  masochistic  helplessness 
saturate  their  "preconscious"  fantasies. 


PSYCHOSOMATIC  PERSONALITY  TYPES 


401 


•MASOCHISTl 

^ThT) 

Figure  45.  The  Mean  Scores  of  Psychotic  Patients  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  and  III-T 
(Hero).  Key:  The  summary  placements  for  each  level  are  based  on  the  indices  em- 
ployed in  Figures  33,  34,  and  35. 

Psychosis  (according  to  the  results  from  this  small  sample)  is  re- 
lated in  many  cases  to  a  desperate  attempt  to  maintain  a  normal,  con- 
ventional, innocent  fagade  in  the  teeth  of  deeper  feelings  of  weakness 
and  worthlessness.  In  other  cases  psychosis  is  characterized  by  a  solid 
multilevel  structure  of  despair  and  distrust.  The  former  are  usually 
called  paranoids,  the  latter  catatonics  or  depressives. 

Clinical  Implications 

The  tentative  implications  to  be  drawn  from  these  results  are  as 
follows:  First,  psychosis  cannot  be  determined  by  the  symptomatic 
pattern  of  the  MMPI.  Psychotics  often  present  themselves  as  hyper- 
normal  at  this  level.  Neither  can  psychotic  diagnosis  be  determined 
by  the  patient's  self-descriptions.    This  group  is  outstanding  in  its 


402         SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

tendency  to  claim  sweet,  congenial  innocence.  Level  III  (i.e.,  the 
TAT)  seems  to  be  a  useful  instrument,  since  it  statistically  separates 
the  psychotics  from  other  groups  (e.g.,  controls  and  dermatitis  pa- 
tients) who  have  a  similar  conventional  fa9ade. 

Whenever  a  patient  presents  a  multilevel  pattern  which  involves 
claimed  conventional  friendliness  contrasted  with  a  distrustful  or  a 
masochistic  Level  III  score,  the  danger  flag  should  be  flown.  This  is 
the  paranoid  phenomenon. 

This  pattern  indicates  poor  motivation  for  therapy.  The  patient 
claims  to  be  a  normal  person  and  probably  does  not  want  his  per- 
sonality investigated  or  changed. 

This  pattern  invariably  involves  projection  of  hostility  and  blame 
onto  others.  The  symptoms  and  the  underlying  pathology  are  not 
consciously  accepted  or  internalized.  Negative  feelings  are  attributed 
to  external  forces.  At  best,  this  forecasts  a  difficult  prognosis.  The  pa- 
tient disclaims  responsibility  for  his  troubles.  It  often  predicts  to  a 
prepsychotic  picture. 

A  second  pattern  typical  of  many  hospitalized  patients  involves  no 
conflict  and  is  characterized  by  solid,  three-level  distrust  and  isola- 
tion. The  clinical  implications  for  this  subgroup  are  quite  different. 
These  patients  are  well  motivated  for  treatment.  They  will  be  willing 
to  admit  and  display  their  guilt  and  passivity.  They  have  a  very  slow 
prognosis  for  change  because  of  the  severity  and  deep-seated  nature 
of  the  self-punitive  feehngs.   They  are  often  interminable  cases. 

References 

1.  Ale.kander,  F.  Fsychosovwtic  medicme.  New  York:  Norton,  1950. 

2.  Alexander,  F.  The  influence  of  psychologic  factors  upon  gastro-intestinal  dis- 
turbances: a  symposium.  I.  General  principles,  objectives  and  preliminary  results. 
Psychoanal.  Quart.,  1934,  3,  501. 

J.  Alvarez,  W.  C.  Ways  in  which  emotion  can  affect  the  digestive  tract.  J. A.M. A., 
1929,  92,  1231. 

4.  Hartman,  H.  R.  Neurogenic  factors  in  peptic  ulcer.  M.  Clm.  North  America, 
1933,  16,  1357. 

5.  Saul,  L.  J.  Hostility  in  cases  of  essential  hypertension.  Psychosom.  Med.,  1939,  I. 
153. 


25 

Analysis  of  Group  Dynamics  in  an 
Industrial  Management  Group 


The  social  behavior  manifested  by  any  individual  represents  his 
method  for  warding  off  anxiety.  Interpersonal  security  operations 
make  the  individual  more  comfortable.  They  also  tend  to  create  the 
social  world  in  which  the  individual  exists  by  means  of  mutual  train- 
ing processes.  Reciprocal  interpersonal  relationships  develop  in  which 
each  partner  trains  the  other  to  respond  in  a  consistent  way.  The 
sado-masochistic  relationship  is  a  common  symbiotic  pairing.  Weak- 
ness and  fear  on  the  part  of  one  pulls  impatience  and  contempt  from 
the  other — which  in  turn  increases  the  fear  of  the  masochist. 

These  symbiotic  relationships  are  extremely  difficult  to  change. 
The  involuntary,  automatic  nature  of  interpersonal  reflexes  makes 
them  almost  impossible  to  control.  The  reinforcing  factor  makes 
them  resistant  to  alter  because  both  partners  are  exerting  pressure  in 
the  same  direction.  A  tight  symbiotic  lock  often  develops  from  which 
neither  partner  can  extricate  himself. 

These  relationships  occur  whenever  human  beings  are  in  con- 
sistent contact  with  each  other.  Marital  and  familial  interpersonal 
linkage  are  the  most  common  subject  matters  for  psychological  study. 
Most  of  the  time  and  energy  expended  in  psychotherapy  is  devoted 
to  understanding  and  loosening  familial  locks. 

Another  most  common  setting  for  rigid  interpersonal  relationships 
is  the  occupational.  Persons  who  work  regularly  together  inevitably 
develop  patterns  of  interaction  which  can  make  for  a  comfortable 
and  productive  job  situation  or  which  can  lead  to  pain,  anxiety,  and 
disorganization. 

One  of  the  tasks  of  the  psychological  consultant  for  industry  is  to 
diagnose  and  help  correct  pathological  interpersonal  patterns  which 
often  exist  in  management  groups.  Before  World  War  II,  the  indus- 

403 


404        SOME  APPUCATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

trial  psychologist  typically  concentrated  on  the  noninterpersonal  fac- 
tors relating  to  productivity — time-and-motion  studies,  intelligence 
and  aptitude  testing,  employee  morale,  etc.  More  recently,  psycho- 
logical consultants  have  discovered  that  personality  characteristics  of 
employees  and  executives  and  group  dynamics  factors  play  a  crucial 
role  in  productivity  and  job  satisfaction.  This  shift  to  a  clinical  ap- 
proach to  industrial  problems  has  been  accompanied  by  a  shift  in  the 
status  level  of  the  subjects  studied.  It  has  become  increasingly  clear 
that  emotional  maladjustment  or  a  rigid  interpersonal  operation  in 
the  case  of  a  top-level  executive  can  initiate  a  pattern  of  destructive 
events  which  can  affect  hundreds  of  people.  In  the  clinic  we  work 
from  the  standpoint  of  the  individual,  and  we  assume  that  the  mal- 
adjustment and  suffering  of  the  individual  is  to  be  treated  regardless  of 
the  status  and  power  level  of  the  patient.  The  psychologist  who  ac- 
cepts the  job  as  consultant  to  an  industrial  firm  or  a  labor  organiza- 
tion often  devotes  most  of  his  energies  to  the  top-management  execu- 
tives. Increased  insight  and  decreased  anxiety  at  the  top  level  usually 
bring  about  a  greater  social  gain.  If  a  union  shop  steward  is  narcissistic 
and  dictatorial,  he  takes  away  a  fraction  of  the  union's  over-all  effi- 
ciency. If  the  regional  director  of  the  union  is  narcissistic  and  dicta- 
torial, he  may  cause  crippHng  reverses  to  his  organization  and  involve 
hundreds  of  subordinates  in  painful  experiences. 

Similarly  if  a  store-to-store  salesman  is  sadistic  and  exploitive,  he 
may  cut  the  firm's  dollar  volume  by  a  few  hundred  dollars.  Sadistic 
and  exploitive  operations  on  the  part  of  the  vice-president  in  charge 
of  sales  may  cost  a  corporation  millions  of  dollars  and  set  up  a  cycle 
of  sado-masochistic  behavior  in  scores  of  people  who  work  under  him. 

In  many  cases  interpersonal  diagnosis  can  assist  the  psychological 
consultant  to  understand  the  misperceptions  of  self  and  others,  the 
rigid  patterns  of  interpersonal  reactions  which  lock  group  members 
in  destructive  relationships.  This  chapter  presents  a  case-history  il- 
lustration of  the  use  of  the  interpersonal  system  in  analyzing  group 
dynamics  in  a  top-level  executive  group. 

The  Top-Management  Groups 

The  group  to  be  discussed  comprised  four  executives  who  were 
responsible  for  the  management  of  a  manufacturing  and  distributing 
plant  of  a  nation-wide  corporation.  Personality  evaluations  were 
made  of  the  top  level  executives  of  this  plant — and  during  the  assess- 
ment process  each  person  took  the  interpersonal  checklist  four  times 
— rating  himself  and  his  perceptions  of  the  other  three  executives  with 

'  Certain  changes  in  the  descriptions  of  this  industrial  organization  were  made  in 
order  to  preserve  anonymity. 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP  405 

whom  he  worked  closely.  The  consulting  psychologist,  after  com- 
pleting his  diagnostic  interviews  rated  each  subject  on  the  interper- 
sonal adjective  check  list. 

The  four  executives  tested  were: 

The  General  Manager  (coded  GM) 
The  Production  A4anager  (coded  PM) 
The  Sales  Manager  (coded  SM) 
The  Personnel  Manager  (coded  LM) 

Interpersonal  tests  provided  several  kinds  of  data  useful  in  under- 
standing the  network  of  relationships  existing  in  this  management 
group.  The  Level  II-C  scores  provide  a  picture  of  how  each  person 
sees  himself.  The  pooled  ratings  of  the  other  three  executives  (plus 
the  psychologist)  provide  for  each  person  a  Level  I-S  measure  of  his 
general  social  stimulus  value.  When  the  ratings  that  each  person 
made  of  each  specific  "other"  are  inspected,  the  patterns  of  misper- 
ception  and  the  reciprocal  relationships  linking  each  pair  to  each 
other  become  obvious. 

Interpersonal  diagnosis  of  the  network  of  interpersonal  relation- 
ships existing  in  groups  is  facilitated  by  the  use  of  a  printed  booklet. 
This  form,  entitled  "Record  Booklet  for  Interpersonal  Analysis  of 
Group  Dynamics,"  was  used  in  the  diagnosis  of  this  management 
group.  The  booklet  of  one  member  (the  Sales  Manager)  of  the 
group  is  presented  as  Figure  48  in  this  chapter.  This  reproduction  of 
the  booklet  outlines  the  exact  operations  for  measuring  the  dynamics 
of  the  subject's  relationships  with  his  colleagues.  The  booklets  of 
the  other  three  executives  are  not  reproduced,  but  the  summary  dia- 
grams of  their  perceptions  of  self  and  others  are  presented  where 
appropriate  to  illustrate  the  text. 

The  Level  II-C  Perceptions  of  Self  by  Four  Executives 

Figure  46  presents  the  Level  II-C  self-perceptions  of  the  four  ex- 
ecutives. All  of  them  see  themselves  as  strong,  hypernormal,  and 
responsible.  All  four  scores  fall  in  the  upper  right-hand  quadrant. 
We  note  that  the  Production  Manager  (PM)  attributes  more  strength 
to  himself,  while  the  Personnel  Manager  (LM)  claims  the  most 
friendliness.  All  of  them  deny  hostihty  and  weakness. 

The  Level  I-S  Ratings  by  Four  Executives 

When  the  ratings  of  each  subject  by  his  three  colleagues  (plus  the 
psychologist)  were  pooled  and  plotted,  a  measure  of  public  stimulus 
value  was  obtained.  Figure  47  presents  these  Level  I-S  scores  for 
each  subject.  This  diagram  also  includes  arrows  linking  the  Level  I-S 


4o6 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


.^^'' 

.^ 


•  PM 


1 

N 

1 

■/ 

\ 

^ 

^ 

\ 

/ 

S    '0 

^°. 

>0 

,,:> 

ir^ 

'0 

•  0 

,,,,' 

^ 

\ 

Jjj-y 

^ 

■"  1 " " 
V 

"^^. 


:£^^^c. 


# 


.^^^ 


.w'^; 


^^^4^ 


'^(^-MASOCHISTIC 


V' 


Figure  46.   Self-Descriptions  of  Four  Executives. 

Key:  GM  =  General  Manager  PM  =  Production  Manager 

SM  =  Sales  Manager  LM  =  Personnel  Manager 

scores  to  the  Level  II-C  self-descriptions.  The  linear  distance  between 
these  two  scores  defines  the  index  of  self-deception.  The  longer  the 
arrow  the  larger  the  misperception,  i.e.,  the  greater  the  discrepancy 
between  the  self-descriptions  and  the  self-as-seen-by-others.  The  di- 
rection of  the  arrow  indicates  what  the  subject  misperceives. 

Figure  47  tells  us  that  the  General  Manager  (GM)  has  the  most  ac- 
curate self-perceptions.  He  is  probably  the  most  effectively  function- 
ing member  of  this  group.  His  colleagues  (and  the  psychologist)  see 
him  as  a  strong  forceful,  nonhostile  person.  They  clearly  admire  and 
respect  him.  There  is  a  minor  misperception  in  that  he  claims  to  be 
more  sympathetic  and  friendly  than  he  acts,  and  he  fails  to  perceive 
some  of  his  bossy  tendencies.  On  the  whole,  he  can  (from  the  psy- 
chological standpoint)  be  considered  a  successful  general  manager. 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


407 


PM 


MANAGEfiMt. 


(AP) 


-^1  ^ 
Q 


^%/, 


^<0, 


%. 


K<9 


/ 

\ 

-^ 

/  ^^ 

/            \ 

\v^^    / 

V 

^ 

\ 

^--n^^X 

/ 

\,---'l 

/ 

!___ " 

1 

0 

«  0 

-r\i 

r; 

— —        6|0 

0               /        80             _            9 

^ 

^ 

\ 

'""1""'   "" 

/ 

V- 

SM^ 

\ 

/ 

[\ 

>v 

/                                               / 

J?\^ 


% 


sf^^A 


£^^>^C; 


LM« 


^[^I^SOCHISTIC 

^ThTT 

Figure  47.  Social  Stimulus  Value  of  Four  Executives,  Plotted  Indices  of  Self-De- 
ception.  Key:  The  labeled  points  (e.g.,  PM)  represent  the  pooled  Level  I-S  behavior 
of  the  subject  as  rated  by  others.  The  arrows  link  the  Level  I-S  score  to  the  subject's 
self-perception.  The  length  of  the  arrow  indicates  how  much  self-deception  exists. 
The  direction  of  the  arrow  indicates  what  the  subject  misperceives. 

The  Production  Manager  (PM)  manifests  a  much  larger  misper- 
ception.  He  is  seen  by  others  as  an  extremely  cold,  hard,  unfriendly, 
selfish  person.  They  fear  him.  They  do  not  like  him.  Not  one 
friendly  or  sympathetic  trait  was  attributed  to  him.  When  we  com- 
pare his  Level  I-S  social  stimulus  value  with  his  self-report  a  grave 
misperception  exists.  He  claims  to  be  strong  and  somewhat  friendly. 
He  completely  denies  the  hostility  and  coldness  which  others  see  in 
him.  This  discrepancy  is  always  diagnostic  of  unsuccessful  inter- 
personal relations  and  poor  emotional  communications.  The  Person- 
nel Manager  believes  he  operates  on  the  basis  of  an  impersonal,  re- 
spected strength.  He  is  probably  puzzled  and  frustrated  when  others 
react  to  him  as  a  conceited  and  sadistic  person.    Harmonious  and 


40 8        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

accurate  relationships  cannot  exist  in  this  group  until  this  mispercep- 
tion  is  corrected. 

The  Sales  Manager  (SM)  is  even  more  self- deceived.  He  is  seen 
by  others  as  a  bitter,  suspicious,  nonconventional  member  of  the  ex- 
ecutive group.  He,  on  the  other  hand,  thinks  of  himself  as  a  friendly, 
affiliative  tolerant  person.  It  is  very  easy  to  deduce  the  confusion  and 
dissatisfaction  vi^hich  occurs  when  he  communicates  with  the  others. 
He  sees  his  ideas  coming  from  a  tolerant,  respected  person.  The  others 
see  his  ideas  coming  from  a  sour,  rebellious,  unfriendly  person.  This 
discrepancy  is  strong  evidence  for  (1)  a  severe  personality  maladjust- 
ment and  (2)  a  chaotic  and  confused  set  of  interpersonal  interactions. 

The  Personnel  Manager  (LM)  is  also  an  inaccurate  judge  of  his 
own  interpersonal  stimulus  value.  He  prides  himself  (at  Level  II-C)  as 
being  a  friendly,  likable,  cooperative  person.  He  is  seen  by  his  col- 
leagues and  the  psychologist  as  a  weak,  dependent  conformist.  It  is 
clear  that  what  he  sees  as  agreeability  is  actually  registered  by  others 
as  slavish  docility.  The  personnel  manager  is  clearly  looked  down 
upon  by  the  others.  They  show  little  respect  for  him. 

Analysis  of  Group  Dynamics 

In  studying  the  over-all  pattern  of  the  group's  interrelationships, 
the  first  point  to  be  noted  is  that  all  members  see  themselves  as  re- 
sponsible or  hypernormal  (octant  8).  This  is  a  consciously  self- 
satisfied  group. 

So  far  as  the  public  observed  behavior  is  concerned  this  group  is 
extremely  centrifugal.  A  wide  variety  of  intense  maladaptive  be- 
havior is  revealed.  No  member  is  diagnosed  by  others  as  responsible 
or  hypernormal.  Their  Level  I-S  diagnoses  are:  autocratic,  narcissistic, 
distrustful,  and  dependent.  Two  members  are  seen  as  being  quite 
hostile.  One  is  seen  as  extremely  weak.  A  pathological  network  of 
interpersonal  relations  invariably  accompanies  extreme  left-hand  and 
bottom  scores.  It  is  possible  to  diagnose  this  as  a  fairly  "sick"  group. 

It  is  moreover  a  most  misperceptive  group.  Three  of  the  members 
fail  by  a  wide  margin  to  perceive  accurately  their  own  interpersonal 
roles.  The  mean  discrepancy  score  (the  linear  distance  in  centimeters 
between  Level  I-S  and  Level  II-C)  can  be  used  as  an  index  of  the 
group's  over-all  tendency  to  misperceive.  This  mean  self-description 
score  can  be  compared  with  the  mean  discrepancy  distance  of  other 
groups  and  a  relative  index  of  group  misperception  is  obtained.  This 
top-level  management  group  has  a  self-deception  score  of  8L^  This 
is  considerably  larger  than  the  mean  of  psychotherapy  groups  com- 

*  The  methods  for  measuring  variability  indices  such  as  self-deception  are  described 
in  Chapter  13. 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP  409 

prised,  for  the  most  part,  of  self-referred  severe  neurotics.  Not  enough 
data  on  management  groups  has  accumulated  to  develop  norms,  but 
it  is  safe  to  say  that  this  group  of  executives  is  considerably  more 
self-deceived  than  the  average. 

The  three  estimates  of  group  dynamics  thus  lead  us  to  diagnose  this 
group  as  disturbed:  (1)  they  exhibit  an  extreme  amount  of  hostility 
or  weakness;  (2)  each  person  sees  himself  as  hypernormal  or  respon- 
sible; and  (3)  they  are  markedly  self-deceived.  Some  form  of  psy- 
chological counseling  is  clearly  in  order. 

The  Network  of  Relationships 

In  addition  to  the  generalized  analysis  of  the  dynamics  of  the  total 
group  (just  presented)  the  sociometric  tests  make  possible  a  detailed 
analysis  of  the  network  of  relationships.  This  is  accomplished  by 
studying  in  turn  how  each  person  rates  each  other  person  in  the  group. 
In  discussing  human  relations  factors  with  these  men  or  in  under- 
standing their  interactions  it  is  most  useful  to  consider  the  pairings 
which  occur. 

The  Sales  Manager 

The  Sales  Manager  was  a  brilhant,  aggressive,  colorful  man — who 
had  been  extremely  successful  in  the  company  before  his  transfer  to 
the  present  location.  His  originality  and  nonconforming  creativeness 
which  had  worked  well  in  the  previous  office  situation  (where  his  in- 
dividuality was  sympathetically  encouraged)  had  degenerated  into  a 
bitter  rebellion.  His  job  frustration  led  to  heavy  drinking,  outbursts  of 
resentment,  and  a  suspicious  distrust  of  his  colleagues. 

The  perceptions  by  the  Sales  Manager  of  his  colleagues  and  their 
perceptions  of  him  were  entered  in  the  "Record  Booklet  for  Inter- 
personal Analysis  of  Group  Dynamics."  This  form  provides  a  syste- 
matic procedure  for  plotting  the  network  of  relationships  in  which 
the  subject  is  engaged.  Figure  48  is  a  reproduction  of  this  booklet 
presenting  the  detailed  operations  for  interpersonal  diagnosis. 

The  upper  diagnostic  circle  in  this  figure  indicates  that  all  observers 
see  the  Sales  Manager  as  a  hostile,  bitter  person.  All  his  Level  I-S 
scores  fall  in  the  DE  and  FG  octants.  The  Personnel  Manager  at- 
tributes more  strength  to  him  than  do  the  rest.  This  is  a  misperception 
and  represents  the  Personnel  Manager's  fearful,  masochistic  tenden- 
cies. The  Production  Manager  sees  the  Sales  A4anager  as  defeated  and 
weak — anything  which  is  unconventional  is  seen  as  weakness  by  the 
Production  Manager.  Note  the  complete  misperception  of  his  own 
behavior — i.e.,  the  difference  between  his  own  and  the  others'  views 
of  him. 


4IO        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

The  lower  diagnostic  circle  in  Figure  48  presents  his  perceptions 
of  the  other  executives.  He  despises  the  Personnel  Manager  whom 
he  sees  as  weak.  He  lumps  the  Production  Manager  and  the  General 
Manager  together  as  unsympathetic,  autocratic  people.  His  failure  to 
differentiate  the  two  represents  a  pessimistic  and  suspicious  feeling  of 
persecution.  He  attributes  no  friendliness  to  any  of  them  (they  are  all 
on  the  left  of  the  vertical  line).  He  lives  in  a  cold,  unloving  environ- 
ment. The  lower  diagnostic  circle  in  Figure  48  also  contains  the 
consensual  diagnosis  of  each  group  member.  These  are  the  lower-case 
letter  codes  (e.g.,  "gm").  The  perceptions  of  the  Sales  Manager  can 
now  be  compared  with  the  consensual  or  pooled  diagnosis.  The  differ- 
ence between  the  two  scores  is  an  index  of  misperception.  The  Sales 
Manager's  perceptions  (capital  letters)  are  linked  by  lines  with  the 
pooled  summary  perception  of  each  other  member  by  the  remaining 
group  members  (lower-case  letters).  This  illustrates  the  variability 
index  of  misperception — i.e.,  the  difference  between  the  Sales  Man- 
ager's view  and  the  consensual  view  by  the  group  of  each  member. 
His  view  of  the  Production  Manager  is  extremely  accurate.  His  view 
of  the  other  two  members  is  quite  inaccurate.  He  attributes  more 
hostility  to  both  members  than  the  consensus. 

Figure  48  reveals  the  following  relationships. 

(1)  The  Sales  Manager  erroneously  sees  the  General  Manager  as 
being  as  hardboiled  as  the  Production  Manager.  This  is  unfortunate 
because  he  cuts  himself  off  from  a  potential  source  of  support.  The 
General  Manager,  we  remember,  prides  himself  (Level  Il-C)  on  his 
benevolence.  The  pressure  of  the  Sales  Manager's  bitter  distrust  in- 
evitably wounds  the  General  Manager's  picture  of  himself.  He  is 
made  anxious  when  he  is  not  being  obeyed  and  respected.  The  Sales 
iManager's  rebelliousness  thus  isolates  him  further. 

(2)  The  Production  Manager  looks  down  on  the  Sales  Manager, 
seeing  him  as  an  extremely  resentful,  complaining  person.  A  loaded 
relationship  exists.  The  Production  Manager  is  perfectly  placed  (on 
the  interpersonal  grid)  to  bring  out  in  exaggerated  form  the  Sales 
Manager's  distrust.  A  vicious  reverberating  circuit  is  set  up  between 
the  two.  The  Production  Manager  feels  justified  because  the  Sales 
Manager  flies  off  on  bitter,  complaining,  suspicious  harangues.  The 
latter  feels  justified  because  the  Personnel  Manager's  superior  scorn 
inevitably  infuriates  him.  Both  men  are  pushing  each  other  further  in 
the  direction  of  their  pathological  reflexes.  Outside  intervention  by 
the  psychologist  or  the  General  Manager  is  clearly  necessary  to  break 
up  this  destructive  lock. 

(3)  The  Sales  Manager's  relationship  with  the  Personnel  Manager 
is  also  destructive.  He  is  a  rebellious  person — made  anxious  by  con- 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP  41 1 

Record  Booklet  For 
Interpersonal  Analysis  of  Group  Dynamics 


<.J.i-,.  jxl:^.**«<»</>*tf^4'                ^^4t4». 

^m 

M     n... 

pt.~.. 

FA..-.:»-       A*    ^£a^S                         ,v„.p„.!^      ^l£fyt^,9A/^^^^                u-:...  c.^..,      ^ 

r.»..p                                                  1  f.»i^ 

Dote  Group 

Dote  Group 

Mseting  time: 

M.fnbe>i  of  Group                                Initlol 

Other  Members  (Arronge  Alphobe. .colly) 

R«»orkt 

Pif^SaA/Ay£^    y^^A/j^^i^^       ^/*^ 

Thit  booklat  wot  prepared  by  Timothy  Laory,  PK.D..  and  published  by 
Rod,    B.rk.l.y   8,    Col.lornid.    Ch«cl.li.f.,    boobl.l.,   I.mpl.t...   norm. 
Piychologtcal  ConiultMion  Scrvic*. 

Figure  48. 

.h.F 

'.ycholo9icol   Con.„l...,on  S„v.c.,    1230  Ou..n. 
i  initruction  menuoll  can   b*  oblsm.d  from  tho 

412        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


413 


e    « 

—     S3 


N     ^ 

5^  s  ►^^ 


^ll 


&  s  s  s 

1  -?  -f  -i 

O  O  O  «l 

S  Z  Z  2 


.2   S 

o    E 
e  _• 


N 

Vo 

»*) 

^ 

6  • 
2  c 

ol 


o .  • : 

.ilx  J- 


414 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


1 

Ur 

\ 

>> 

Is 

^  m 

O 

-J 

^ 

o 

? 

1 

III 

S 

Opera- 
tional 
Deflni. 
lion  of 
Index 

^J 

J 

J 

1 

J 

1 
1 

1 
1 

1 
1 

ill 

s 

il 

m 

c 
o 

II 
111 

c 
o 

111 

II 

all 

c 
.2 

.2  - 

111 

1 

i 

c 

0 

Si 

c 

Ut 

iJ 

II 
It 

il 

It 
II 

•S3. 

ill 
111 

ill 

fen   Si  w 

J-o5  8 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^  1 

"1 

-1 

5; 

f 

1 

^ 

^ 

^ 

? 

7 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

ill 

> 
^ 

Si 

^ 

^ 

^ 

!« 

illiJ 

5 

> 

^ 

f 

i 

J 

i2-l 

J 

1 

ill 

•2  =  o 

JJJ 

111 

c 

S 

i 

GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


4^5 


1 
1 

1 

X 

J 

J 

J 

it 

c 
o 

h 

c 
.2 

IS 

t 

s 

ll 
15 

Misperception  of  Subject  by  Others 

This  misperception  index  is  the  distance  between  X  and  the  capi- 
tal letter  summary  of  the  appropriate  fellow  group  member  from 
Figure  1.  Fill  in  the  blank  with  the  appropriate  capital  letter 

^ 

§ 

^ 

^ 

1 

^ 

1 

t 

^ 

^ 

! 

^ 

^ 

1 

1 

^ 

^J 

c 
o 

m 

% 

p. 
IS 

1 

s 

h 

si 

^5 


II 

5S  ^ 


^ 


>>      >> 

c      c 


S  I -J 

2    f^  2. 


c 

1 

c 

I 

§ 

^ 

\ 

I 

^ 

k 

! 

1 

s 

! 

is 


ii 


:2      t    ^ 

—      •    IP. 


^i 


n       w       M 


1     1 


■B  5 
o 

■ll 


s   s   s 


4i6 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


^      a. 

±     8 


in 

0 

X 

-J 

^ 

^ 

S) 

i§ 

X 

S 
^ 

p 

^ 

9 

^ 

X 

X 

-J 

^ 

1 

5; 

V) 

^ 

^ 

1 

Total 
Item. 
Check- 
ed 

>- 
z 

:§ 

■5 
3 

•5 
t 

1 

m 

fv 

•P 

^ 

- 

M 

<N 

- 

• 

IS 

s 

i 

1 

i 

s 

^ 

^ 

^ 

M 

i 

II 

■5  I 
3^ 

1 

0 
z 

1 

J* 

1 
1 

•0 

ri 

5i 

1 

1 

1 

! 

*0 

*0 

u5 

«6 

CO 

GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


417 


X 
-J 

^ 

5! 

^ 

■s 

c  0 

0  u 

10 

^ 

^ 

N 

Member 
igure  2) 
[Consider 

"o-o 

tS5 

>0 

> 

^ 

1 

of  Each 

ns  for  F 

Member 

2 

^ 

§ 

? 

i  Group 

Locatic 
let  and 

3 

I 
J 

^ 

1 

T 

1 

itions  b 
1  Letter 
of  Book 

</5 

\ 

1 

Summed  Percei 

(Providing  Sma 
Excluding  Subject 

c 

1 

I 

"a 
I.. 

6-0 

1 

^ 
^ 

:i 

i 

z 

1 

1 

o    o  ^-> 
3     «n     ^ 

o^u. 


S;  ^   o 


-0  „ 

X 
-J 

K 

^ 

^ 

^ 

y 

^1 

Vo 

k      J 

1 

2^ 

>/> 

^ 

<^ 

Pn 

V 

"i? 

Li 

>5^ 

^ 

•^ 

A 

^ 

X 

^ 

N 

On 

5 

-1 

1^ 

>? 

? 

» 

CO 

N. 

^ 

v> 

tt 

0 

\ 

f 

? 

•A 
1 

\ 

1 

^ 

• 

>• 

• 

_ 

_ 

■% 

>■ 

>. 

>> 

>• 

>. 

3 

Z        ' 

in 

ffi 

CO 

CO 

OQ 

CQ 

m 

H 

1-   z 

4i8 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


formity  and  blind  obedience.  The  Personnel  Manager,  who  epitomizes 
these  docile  interpersonal  reflexes,  stands  as  a  continual  annoyance  and 
threat  to  him.  He  responds  to  the  Personnel  Manager  with  contempt 
and  sarcasm.  The  Personnel  Manager  is  obviously  afraid  of  the  Sales 
Manager,  describing  him  as  aggressive  and  sadistic. 

The  General  Manager 

The  General  Manager  is  a  relatively  young  executive,  well-trained 
in  both  the  technical  and  managerial  aspects  of  his  job.  He  had  made 
a  rapid  advance  in  the  corporation  and  is  about  ten  years  younger  than 
the  Production  and  Personnel  managers. 

Figure  49  presents  his  perceptions  of  his  colleagues.  He  shares  the 
consensus  view  of  the  Production  Manager,  seeing  him  as  cold  and 


/  i? 

Q 


MANAGEKMt 


i4p; 


'^^r. 


X 


%. 


.      "         M 

;^ 

,  ,,?, 

80     LM    , 

'-^. 

\Vx 

/ 

M 


If. 


^^f. 


Figure  49.  The  Perceptions  by  the  General  Manager  of  His  Three  CoUeagues. 

Key:  PM  =  The  General  Manager's  description  of  the  Production  Manager 
SM  =  The  General  Manager's  description  of  the  Sales  Manager 
LM  =  The  General  Manager's  description  of  the  Personnel  Manager 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP  419 

self-centered.  He  respected  his  technical  abilities  but  disapproved  of 
(and  somewhat  feared)  his  cold,  impersonal,  conceited  approach. 

He  has  mixed  feelings  towards  the  Sales  Manager.  He  attributes 
less  hostility  to  him  than  do  any  of  the  other  raters  (see  Figure  48) 
and  bends  over  backwards  to  understand  and  tolerate  the  Sales 
Manager's  rebelliousness. 

The  General  Manager  clearly  likes  the  Personnel  Manager.  He 
sees  him  as  cooperative  and  agreeable.  This  is  a  misperception.  The 
rest  of  the  raters  see  the  Personnel  Manager  as  a  docile,  weak  person. 
The  Personnel  Manager  placates  and  submits  and  flatters  the  General 
Manager,  winning  the  latter's  approval. 

The  General  Manager  tends  to  "stress  the  positive"  in  his  approach 
to  the  Sales  Manager  and  the  Personnel  Manager — failing  to  see  the 
bitterness  of  the  former  and  the  weakness  of  the  latter. 

Figure  50  summarizes  the  perceptions  of  the  General  Manager  by 
his  colleagues  and  the  psychologist.  This  diagram  indicates  that  the 
Production  Manager  attributes  the  least  amount  of  strength  to  the 
General  Manager.  He  views  him  as  too  easygoing.  We  suspect  that 
the  Production  Manager  feels  somewhat  superior  to  his  boss  and 
thinks  he  is  too  soft.  The  Personnel  Manager  tends  to  idolize  his  boss. 
He  attributes  strength  and  responsibility  to  him.  The  Sales  Manager 
assigns  more  hostility  to  the  General  Manager  than  any  other  rater. 
This  indicates  that  he  feels  somewhat  resentful  and  misunderstood 
by  his  boss.  The  psychologist  emphasizes  the  executive  autocratic 
traits  of  the  General  Manager. 

The  data  from  Figures  49  and  50  can  now  be  combined  in  a  series 
of  statements  summarizing  the  General  Manager's  interpersonal  re- 
lationships. 

(1)  He  is  involved  in  an  uneasy  truce  with  the  Production  Man- 
ager. He  respects  but  disapproves  of  the  latter's  coldness.  The  Pro- 
duction Manager  respects  but  looks  down  on  the  General  Manager 
and  feels  he  is  too  soft  and  tolerant. 

(2)  He  is  involved  in  an  authority  problem  with  the  Sales  Man- 
ager. He  strives  to  be  sympathetic  and  benign  in  his  approach  to  his 
touchy,  rebeUious  subordinate,  but  this  does  not  prevent  the  latter 
from  distrustfully  projecting  coldness  on  him.  These  two  need  to  be 
helped  to  clarify  their  mutual  misperceptions.  The  General  Manager 
is  not  as  self-centered  and  exploitive  as  the  Sales  Manager  fears. 

( 3  )  He  is  locked  in  a  tight  and  mutually  self-deceptive  relationship 
with  the  Personnel  Manager.  They  both  try  to  believe  that  they  have 
a  collaborative,  friendly  union  of  equals.  Actually  an  intense  leader- 
follower  association  exists.  The  Personnel  Manager  fails  to  see  how 
passive  and  placating  he  is  with  his  boss,  although  all  three  of  the  other 


420 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


\ 


^Y% 


.^^^A 


?^^C; 


"''<^-«/(SOCHlSTlC 


Figure  50.  The  Perceptions  o^  the  General  Manager  by  His  Three  Colleagues  and 
the  Consulting  Psychologist.  Key;  The  General  Manager  as  seen  by: 


PM  =  Production  Manager 
SM  —  Sales  Manager 
LM  =  Personnel  Manager 
C  =  Psychological  Consultant 
T  =  Pooled  scores  of  all  four 


observers  are  aware  of  this  (see  Figure  54).  The  General  Manager 
fails  to  perceive  how  autocratic  and  paternalistic  he  is  towards  the 
Personnel  Manager.  A  symbiotic,  mutually  self-deceptive  relation- 
ship of  this  sort  can  proceed  indefinitely  in  harmony — except  for  the 
impact  it  has  on  others.  The  Production  Manager  was  contemptuous 
of  both  of  them — and  the  Sales  Manager  was  jealous  of  the  approval 
obtained  by  the  Personnel  Manager  (whom  he  saw  as  a  mollycoddle) 
and  resentful  that  the  General  Manager  did  not  approve  of  him. 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


421 


The  Production  Manager 

The  Production  Manager  is  a  firm,  rigid,  self-confident,  self-made 
man  of  the  old  school.  He  had  no  understanding  or  patience  for  the 
soft-headed  management  policies  which  were  developing  in  his  com- 
pany. Human  relations  bored  and  irritated  him.  He  expected  people 
to  behave  with  the  efficiency  of  his  machinery  and  despised  any 
deviations  from  custom  or  rule. 

Figure  51  reflects  the  superiority  he  felt  over  his  colleagues.  He 
rates  all  of  them  below  the  horizontal  line — which  means  he  sees  none 
of  them  as  strong.  He  is  particularly  contemptuous  of  the  Sales  Man- 
ager and  Personnel  Manager.   He  saw  the  former's  unconventional, 


MANAGEfiMt- 


.^^^ 


^^ 


^UTOC, 


'%// 


'6M 


>SM 


"^/. 


J^O 


■^f^r. 


% 


LM 


12^;£;MAS0CH1ST\C 
^(HlJ 


-#-^ 


Figure  51 

Key:  SM 


The  Perceptions  by  the  Production  Manager  of  His  Three  Colleagues. 


The  Production  Manager's  description  of  the  Sales  Manager 
LM  =The  Production  Manager's  description  of  the  Personnel  Manager 
GM  =  The  Production  Manager's  description  of  the  General  Manager 


42  2 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


creative  rebelliousness  as  a  woolly-headed  complaining.  He  saw  the 
latter's  docility  as  abject  slavishness. 

Figure  52  tells  us  that  everyone  agrees  in  labeling  the  Production 
Manager  as  a  cold,  stubborn,  hardboiled  person.  Even  the  psychol- 
ogist feared  him — which  is  perhaps  due  to  the  Production  Manager's 
outspoken  disdain  for  psychological  consultation. 

Summarizing  the  data  from  Figures  51  and  52  we  can  see  that  the 
Production  Manager  is  involved  in  the  following  relationships: 

( 1 )  He  looks  down  somewhat  on  the  General  Manager,  but  likes 
him  and  recognizes  his  warmth.  His  rigid  commitment  to  order  and 


^^'''- 


y^: 


SM 


GM 


•  LM 


•T 


•  C 


/ 

\^          '^   \ 

■  -^ 

y 

\ 

N^    V 

/ 

^ 

^ — "^n 

/  \v  \ 

/ 

— ' 

1 

0     .     ,            " 

JO 

,,,,«o   ,^Nj 

to= 

■      6|5 

.  .    .  .'.0.  . 

•0             .            9 

__^ 

\         /. 

\ 

r-^ 

■\ 

\    / 

\ 

>V 

/ 

% 


Si2 


^'rA 


f!J^SOCHISTlC 


.# 

^^^"i^^ 


Figure  52.  The  Perceptions  of  the  Production  Manager  by  His  Three  Colleagues 
and  the  Consulting  Psychologist.  Key:  The  Production  Manager  as  seen  by: 

SM  =  Sales  Manager 
LM  =  Personnel  Manager 
GM  =  General  Manager 

C  =  Psychological  Consultant 

T  =  Pooled  scores  of  all  four 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP 


423 


hierarchy  lead  him  to  accept  the  General  Manager  as  boss.  Thus  an 
uneasy  but  effective  relationship  was  maintained.  It  i^  clear  that  the 
best  relationship  the  Production  Manager  has  is  with  the  General  Man- 
ager. If  anyone  is  going  to  get  close  to  the  Production  Manager  and 
relax  his  tough,  hard-headed  approach,  it  is  going  to  be  the  General 
Manager — and  not  the  other  two  men. 

(2)  The  Production  Manager  is  involved  in  a  sadistic  relationship 
with  both  the  other  two  executives.  They  both  fear  and  hate  him. 
The  Sales  Manager  rebelliously  fights  back  but  can  be  provoked  to  ir- 
rational resentment  by  the  Production  Manager's  calm,  punitive  scorn. 

(3)  The  Personnel  Manager  masochistically  submits  to  the  Pro- 
duction Manager.    The  latter's  unsympathetic  hardness  made  him 


Q 


.^"f" 
%>:,#. 


MANAGEflMt.. 


X4P; 


vAV 


•  SM 


% 


PM 


f^r/c 


/^o 


'I'^A, 


6M 


2jC-AMS0CHtST\C 

^(HT) 


% 


/ 

\ 

•k                                  -T 

^ 

/ 

\ 

^V           \- 

/ 

\ 



_^-^ 

-A 

1         ^v,       \ 

/ 

'        ' 

"  ,  ,  ,  1  ,   ,  ,  ,  M  ,  ,  , 

30 

'1°           ^N 

/ 

"610 

1 . 1  iTi . 

80 

_..,^-- 

-vT/ 

\ 

'   "   "1   "   " 

i--— """^ 

\  / 

\ 

/'\ 

/ 

Figure  53.  The  Perceptions  by  the  Personnel  Manager  of  His  Three  Colleagues. 

Key:  GAl  =  The  Personnel  Manager's  description  of  the  General  Manager 

PM  =  The  Personnel  Manager's  description  of  the  Production  Manager 
SM  =  The  Personnel  Manager's  description  of  the  Sales  Manager 


424 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


more  self-effacing  and  timid.  The  Personnel  Manager  was  comforted, 
however,  by  his  slavish  devotion  to  the  General  Manager — who  pro- 
tected him.  The  Sales  Manager  was  isolated  in  impotent  rebellion, 
having  no  positive  relationship  in  the  group. 

The  Fersormel  Manager 

The  Personnel  Manager  was  a  cheerful,  bland,  rather  dull  person. 
Placating  conformity  to  strength  and  friendly  sympathy  to  subordi- 
nates were  his  major  security  operations.  He  was  fairly  successful  in 
his  work  because  of  his  popularity,  and  good  humor.  His  relationships 


►SM 


.^^^? 


^^^^A 


J--0 

•PM 


2!^S0CH1ST1C 


C» 


^9 


Figure  54.  The  Perceptions  oj  the  Personnel  Manager  by  His  Three  Colleagues 
and  the  Consulting  Psychologist.  Key:  The  Personnel  Manager  as  seen  by: 

SM  =  Sales  Manager 
GM  =  General  Manager 
PM  =  Production  Manager 

C  =  Psychological  Consultant 

T  =  Pooled  scores  of  all  four 


GROUP  DYNAMICS  IN  A  MANAGEMENT  GROUP  425 

with  union  officials  were  good  because  he  was  patient,  agreeable  and 
smiled  when  frustrated. 

Figure  5  3  presents  his  perceptions  of  others.  He  admires  the  Gen- 
eral Manager.  He  is  in  fearful  awe  of  the  two  other  men. 

Figure  54  indicates  that  all  raters  see  him  as  weak.  The  General 
Manager  clearly  likes  him  and  an  aura  of  good  feehng  blinds  him  to 
the  Personnel  Manager's  submissiveness.  The  Production  and  Sales 
executives  perceive  him  as  weak,  and  sado-masochistic  relationships 
exist  with  both. 

The  specific  network  of  interactions  involving  the  Personnel  Man- 
ager has  been  presented  in  the  previous  discussions  and  need  not  be 
repeated. 

Summary 

Sociometric  analysis  of  a  top-level  management  group  revealed  that 
considerable  psychopathology  complicated  and  hampered  relation- 
ships. The  misperceptions  and  the  rigid  destructive  symbiotic  interac- 
tions prevented  these  men  from  clarifying  or  improving  their  rela- 
tionships. 

The  psychologist  was  called  in  initially  by  the  General  Manager 
to  consult  about  the  Sales  Manager's  drinking.  Sociometric  analysis  of 
the  group  dynamics  quickly  revealed  that  the  drinking  and  rebellious- 
ness of  the  Sales  Manager  was  not  an  isolated  symptom  but  intimately 
interwoven  into  the  interpersonal  fabric  of  the  top-level  group. 


26 


Predicting  and  Measuring  Interpersonal 
Dynamics  in  Group  Psychotherapy 


The  prediction  of  the  individual's  behavior  in  group  psychotherapy 
was  one  of  the  first  tasks  approached  by  the  interpersonal  system  ( 1 ) . 
In  Chapter  7  and  in  Appendix  1  the  use  of  Level  I-P  indices  as  a  fore- 
cast of  the  patient's  future  role  is  discussed. 

Predicting  the  Resistance  of  the  Group 

These  measures  which  forecast  individual  behavior  have  a  further 
use  in  predicting  the  behavior  to  be  expected  from  the  group  as  a 
group.  It  has  long  been  recognized  that  any  group  is  more  than  an 
additive  assemblage  of  its  parts.  When  the  Level  I-Predictor  scores 
of  the  constituent  members  are  plotted  on  the  same  diagnostic  grid, 
a  network  of  interactions  is  apparent.  It  is  possible  to  predict  from 
this  master  grid  what  interpersonal  operations  will  characterize  this 
group. 

After  working  with  therapy  groups  for  several  years,  the  staff  of 
the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic  found  that  it  was  possible  to  describe 
the  "personality"  of  a  group;  or  to  designate  the  group  resistance.  One 
group,  for  example,  was  anecdotally  described  as  "slow  and  soggy"; 
another  was  labeled  "bitter  and  resistive";  and  another  was  called 
"centrifugal  and  lively." 

Development  of  the  Level  I-S  sociometric  indices  made  it  possible 
to  objectify  these  clinical  intuitions.  In  the  "soggy"  group,  for  ex- 
ample, five  out  of  six  patients  were  rated  below  the  mean  on  domi- 
nance. Their  passivity  and  lethargy  were  clearly  defined  by  plotting 
all  the  Level  1-S  scores  on  the  same  group-diagnostic  grid. 

Development  of  the  MMPI  predictor  indices  made  it  possible  to 
plot  the  expected  role  behavior  of  each  member  of  a  prospective 
group  on  a  master  grid.  The  therapist  then  could  anticipate  the  type 

426 


PREDICTING  BEHAVIOR  IN  GROUP  THERAPY  427 

of  group  resistance.  If  a  large  majority  of  the  Level  I-P  scores  fell  on 
the  left  side  of  the  circle,  then  a  hostile,  defiant,  bitter  group  with 
plenty  of  interpersonal  fireworks  was  forecast.  If  most  of  the  scores 
fell  on  the  right  side,  a  friendly,  sweet,  repressive  resistance  to  treat- 
ment could  be  expected. 

Selecting  Patients  To  Balance  the  Group  Resistance 

The  introduction  of  the  predictive  indices  and  the  methods  for 
measuring  group  resistance  made  it  possible  to  plot  on  group  grids 
the  members  of  some  fort)^-  psychotherapy  groups  studied  by  the  inter- 
personal system.  The  therapists  of  these  groups  were  often  able  to 
detect  from  these  charts  the  group  resistance  and  to  observe  where 
imbalances  in  the  group  put  intense  pressure  on  the  therapist  which 
increased  tension  and  lowered  effectiveness. 

Several  large-scale  studies  of  interpersonal  behavior  and  changes 
during  psychotherapy  have  been  accomplished  by  the  Kaiser  Founda- 
tion research  project.  One  conclusion  reached  is  that  the  more  im- 
balanced  or  homogeneous  the  group  the  less  easy  (and  probably  the 
less  effective)  the  therapy.  By  this  we  mean  that  if  a  majority  of  the 
members  of  a  group  utilize  the  same  interpersonal  security  operations, 
the  task  of  the  therapist  is  considerably  compHcated.  If  five  members 
of  a  group  are  bland  hysterics,  they  will  tend  to  reinforce  each 
other's  reflexes;  they  will  all  like  each  other;  they  will  all  collaborate 
in  denying  and  avoiding  unpleasant  emotions.  The  task  of  the  therapist 
in  facing  a  solid  wall  of  group  repression  can  be  discouraging.  The 
group  joins  together  to  put  the  same  interpersonal  pressure  on  the 
therapist.  If,  however,  a  wise-cracking  psychopath,  a  colorful  exhi- 
bitionistic  narcissist,  and  a  self-immolating  masochist  should  be  added, 
the  pressure  on  the  therapist  is  relieved.  Intense  interactions  develop 
between  the  latter  three  and  the  hysterics.  The  therapist  can  sit  back 
and  observe,  or  intervene  with  technical  activities  without  bearing 
the  brunt  of  a  unified  resistance. 

For  these  reasons  the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinic  has  initiated  a  pro- 
cedure for  selecting  the  members  of  a  therapy  group  so  as  to  provide 
a  balanced  combination  of  Level  I  security  operations.  Patients  re- 
ferred to  groups  by  the  weekly  clinic  intake  conferences  are  placed 
on  a  waiting  list.  When  eight  to  ten  names  are  obtained  the  selection 
procedure  begins.  The  Level  I-P  indices  for  all  the  patients  on  the 
waiting  list  are  plotted  on  one  diagnostic  grid.  The  therapist  can  de- 
termine at  a  glance  whether  the  candidates  pile  up  in  one  sector  or 
scatter  around  the  circle.  If  the  latter  is  the  case,  he  selects  two  pa- 
tients from  each  quadrant  and  these  eight  patients  are  assigned  to  the 
group.    If  the  waiting  list  population  is  overweighted  in  one  direc- 


42  8        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

tion,  the  therapist  postpones  the  group  until  more  patients  are  referred 
to  group  therapy  who  fit  the  absent  "slots."  For  example,  if  six  out  of 
the  eight  candidates  fall  below  the  horizontal  line,  a  passive,  dependent 
group  is  forecast.  The  therapist  would  immediately  select  the  two 
strong  candidates — then  he  would  pick  out  four  of  the  six  passive  pa- 
tients and  postpone  the  group  until  two  more  strong  patients  had  en- 
tered the  waiting  list. 

Selecting  patients  for  group  therapy  thus  becomes  similar  to  the 
casting  of  characters  in  a  play.  The  therapist  by  use  of  predictive 
indices  attempts  to  set  up  a  heterogeneous  group  in  which  interaction 
will  be  maximized. 

Illustration  of  the  Prediction  of  Group  Resistance 

Here  is  an  illustration  of  the  predictive  indices  employed  to  fore- 
cast group  resistance.  There  were  six  members  of  this  psychotherapy 
group.  The  Level  I-P  index  of  each  patient  before  therapy  was  plotted 
on  a  master  grid  (see  Figure  55).  This  group  is  well-balanced  in 
respect  to  conventional  versus  unconventional  operations — three 
members  falling  on  either  side  of  the  vertical  mid-line.  The  group  is 
overweighted,  however,  on  the  dominance  axis.  Only  one  member 
is  going  to  be  passive — five  are  above  the  mean  in  dominance. 

A  noisy,  power-oriented,  self-confident  set  of  reflexes  can  be  an- 
ticipated. The  group  resistance  is  through  strength  and  self-assurance. 
There  will  be  a  pronounced  tendency  for  the  members  to  lecture  each 
other,  debate,  solve  each  other's  problems,  and  compete  for  the  role 
of  group  leader. 

We  can  expect  that  patients  "B"  and  "S"  will  bluntly  press  the 
others  to  express  unconventional  feelings.  Patients  "C,"  "P,"  and  "U" 
will  maintain  a  fagade  of  "hypernormal"  control  and  reasonability. 
Patient  "M"  will  be  isolated  in  his  passivity  and  sit  on  the  side  lines. 
She  is  the  only  member  to  fall  below  the  center  line. 

This  group  will  not  emphasize  (in  the  early  sessions)  the  presenta- 
tion of  problems.  There  will  be  a  minimum  of  dependent,  helpless  be- 
havior. If  the  therapist  attempts  to  intervene,  he  will  find  himself  in 
a  power  struggle.  The  one  area  of  potential  interaction  is  the  differ- 
ence in  conventionality.  Patients  "B"  and  "S"  will  attack  and  chal- 
lenge the  others  to  produce  negative  feelings  (with  which  they  feel 
comfortable).  Patients  "P"  and  "U"  will  take  on  strong,  executive 
roles.  Patient  "C  will  stress  hypernormal  activities  and  blandly  resist 
the  colorful  maneuvers  of  "B"  and  "S."  But  all  five  of  these  patients 
will  act  decisively  and  strongly  as  though  they  know  how  the  therapy 
should  be  run — two  by  blunt  uncovering  of  feelings,  one  by  re- 
pressive denial  of  feelings,  two  by  a  mixture  of  both. 


PREDICTING  BEHAVIOR  IN  GROUP  THERAPY 


429 


%A 


% 


c« 


fj/ 

\ 

~\ 

'"  \ 

""^ 

K 

—    / 

CO 

•s 

/^ 

y 

•    / 

\ 

■ — 

1 

/ 

/ 

ts    '  0 

20                             30 

<o 

^N, 

Irr: 

tlo 

ro                    « 

0                   9 

\ 

\ 

/ 

t 

1"" 

1  •  1  1  '  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1  1 

/ 

-K%^ 


'€. 


^J^AC. 


^1^ 


i^[^SOCHIST\C 

^(Hi) 


..-"' 
^'^t^^ 


Figure  55.  Predictions  of  Interpersonal  Roles  for  Six  Members  of  a  Therapy 
Group. 

Illustration  of  the  Measurement  of  Group  Resistance 

After  this  therapy  group  had  met  for  seven  sessions,  a  sociometric 
was  administered.  Each  patient  rated  himself  and  every  other  patient 
on  the  interpersonal  check  Hst.  The  ratings  by  the  group  of  Patient 
"P"  were  pooled,  the  indices  calculated,  divided  by  five  (N-1),  con- 
verted to  standard  scores,  and  plotted  on  the  diagnostic  grid.  The 
same  procedure  was  applied  to  the  group's  pooled  perceptions  of  each 
other  patient.  This  is  the  Level  I-S  score — a  summary  of  the  social 
impact  of  each  patient  on  his  fellow  members.  These  scores  tell  us 
how  the  patient  acted  in  the  group  and  can  be  compared  with  our 
original  predictions  of  how  the  Level  I-P  indices  predicted  he  would 
act. 

Figure  56  presents  the  Level  I-S  scores  for  the  six  patients  in  this 
group.  The  Level  I-P  predictor  scores  are  also  plotted  and  hnked  to 


430 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


.e5>;a^^^^ 


LX4P; 


^^"^ 


Q/ 


'^^r 
%. 


<?K%^ 


J^o 


-22^/IS0CHIST\C 


Figure  56.  Measurements  of  Interpersonal  Roles  for  Six  Members  of  a  Therapy 
Group.  Key:  Letter  placements  refer  to  Level  I-S  ratings  of  interpersonal  behavior. 
The  nonlabeled  points  to  which  they  are  joined  designate  the  predictions  of  their 
role  (Level  I-P). 


the  Level  I-S  scores  to  indicate  the  amount  of  error  in  the  prediction. 
When  we  consider  the  over-all  pattern  of  the  Level  I-S  scores,  it 
is  clear  that  the  original  prediction  of  group  resistance  was  fairly  ac- 
curate. Four  of  the  patients  are  rated  to  the  left  of  the  vertical  mid- 
line, indicating  that  they  were  seen  as  more  hostile  than  friendly.  The 
prediction  of  group  resistance  was  thus  slightly  in  error  on  the  love- 
hate  axis.  The  forecast  was  more  effective  as  regards  to  dominance- 
submission — since  all  six  patients  were  rated  on  the  side  of  the  hori- 
zontal mid-line  which  matches  the  prediction. 

Multilevel  Reciprocal  Interpersonal  Relationships 

Whenever  a  sociometric  employing  the  interpersonal  adjective 
check  list  has  been  given  to  the  members  of  a  group,  it  is  possible  to 


PREDICTING  BEHAVIOR  IN  GROUP  THERAPY 


431 


plot  the  person-to-person  network  of  relationships.  There  are  two 
group-dynamics  diagrams  drawn  up  for  each  member  using  the 
printed  booklet  which  was  introduced  in  the  preceding  chapter.  One 
of  these  indicates  the  subject's  perceptions  of  every  other  member. 
The  second  indicates  how  the  subject  is  seen  by  each  other  group 
member.  Misperceptions,  reciprocal  role  relationships,  pairings,  and 
isolates  become  easily  apparent  on  these  grids.  This  procedure  for 
analyzing  group  dynamics  was  followed  for  the  therapy  group  being 
described  in  this  chapter.  The  use  of  these  diagrams  in  understanding 
blocks  and  projections  which  compUcate  interpersonal  relations  has 
been  illustrated  in  Chapter  23  (the  management  group)  and  need  not 
be  repeated  in  the  case  of  this  therapy  group. 

There  is,  however,  an  additional  technique  for  analyzing  group 
dynamics  which  involves  the  use  of  multilevel  personality  indices. 
These  methods  are  more  applicable  in  studying  therapy  groups  (rather 
than  industrial  management  groups)  because  measurements  of  under- 
lying feelings  are  available  for  the  psychotherapy  patients  and  because 
the  group  therapy  situation  is  more  suitable  for  discussion  of  deeper 
motivations. 

This  more  complex  analysis  of  multilevel  interaction  patterns  is  ac- 
complished by  plotting  the  patient's  scores  for  Level  L  and  II  Self, 
Mother,  Father,  and  Ideal  and  Level  III  Hero  and  Other  on  the  same 
diagnostic  grid  with  his  perceptions  of  his  fellow  group  members, 
With  these  data  available,  it  is  possible  to  determine  what  meaning  the 
perceptions  of  any  specific  group  member  has  in  relation  to  the  eight 
scores  from  the  subject's  personality  structure. 

If  the  subject  sees  a  fellow  group  member  as  being  close  to  his  own 
self-perception  he  is  consciously  identified  with  him. 

If  he  sees  a  fellow  group  member  as  close  to  his  ideal,  he  is  con- 
sidered to  idealize  him. 

If  he  sees  the  other  as  close  to  his  mother — the  process  of  maternal 
equation  is  indicated.  The  proximity  to  father  or  spouse  scores  is 
similarly  interpreted. 

If  the  subject  perceives  a  fellow  group  member  as  being  close  to 
his  own  Level  III  Hero — a  cross-level  identification  is  defined.  That 
is,  he  projects  onto  or  attributes  to  the  other  member  his  own  "pre- 
conscious"  feelings. 

The  relationship  pattern  of  the  group  is  thus  related  to  the  famihal 
pattern  and  to  the  patient's  preconscious  imagery. 

To  illustrate  this  technique  for  analyzing  multilevel  reciprocal  re- 
lationships we  shall  consider  two  patients  in  the  sample  therapy  group 
who  became  locked  together  in  a  complex  network.  Figure  57  pre- 
sents diagnostic  grids  for  two  patients  "M"  and  "P."  On  each  diagram 


43^ 


SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 


S>^^'^'\ 


HANAGERML- 


v^>:2n^^^. 


.aV 


.v^^ 


•I 


/  i? 

o 


AUToc/ 


■%/, 


''Co: 


'I 


•Sp 


H 


'^f'r. 


^!f!j^SOCHISllC 

PATIENT   "p" 

Figure  57.  Illustrative  Diagram  of  Five  Measures  of  Personality  for  Two  Group- 
Therapy  Patients.  Key:  For  both  patients  I  =  Level  I-S;  II  =  Level  II-C  Self;  III  = 
Level  III-T  Hero.  For  Patient  "P,"  Sp  =  Husband;  "M"  =  "P's"  perception  of  "M." 
For  Patient  "M,"  Mo  =  Mother,  "P"  =  Pauent  "M's"  description  of  "P." 

we  have  plotted  five  indices:  four  personality  scores  for  the  patient 
and  his  perceptions  of  the  other  one.  For  didactic  purposes,  we  have 
omitted  the  other  four  personality  scores  and  the  patient's  perceptions 
of  the  other  group  members.  For  Patient  "P"  we  have  tallied  the 
scores  for  Level  I-S  Self,  Level  II-C  Self,  Level  II-C  Spouse,  the  Level 
III-T  (Hero)  and  the  perception  by  "P"  of  her  partner  "M."  For 
Patient  "M"  five  scores  are  plotted:  Level  I-S  Self,  Level  II-C  Self, 
Level  II-C  Mother,  Level  III-T  (Hero)  and  Patient  "M's"  view  of 
"P." 

Consider  the  diagram  for  Patient  "P."  She  sees  herself  (II)  as  in- 
dependent and  forceful.  She  is  seen  by  the  group  (I)  somewhat  the 


PREDICTING  BEHAVIOR  IN  GROUP  THERAPY 


433 


^X       P^       Mo 


HE 


ISap) 

MANAGERMt-T^ 


§h' 


3?-\'3: 


^% 


%. 


'^r/c 


^<r; 


.^'^^A 


f^^^, 


//v. 


fJ^^OCHISTlC 

^7h7) 


PATIENT      M 


same  way,  although  they  see  more  competitive  narcissism.  She  main- 
tained an  aloof,  cool,  poised  superiority  in  the  group — never  admit- 
ting any  faults  or  weaknesses.  She  continually  patronized  the  other 
patients,  looked  down  on  their  problems  and  smugly  beat  off  any  at- 
tempt to  prove  she  was  not  "right,  wise,  and  capable." 

She  described  her  husband  (Sp)  as  being  a  rebellious,  bitter  man. 
Actually  she  was  in  the  group  because  of  a  marital  problem.  Her  hus- 
band drank,  gambled,  and  beat  her  in  drunken  rages.  Her  stated  rea- 
son for  accepting  group  therapy  was  to  understand  herself,  her  role 
in  the  marriage,  and  to  learn  how  "to  handle"  her  husband  more  ef- 
fectively. 

The  deeper  reasons  for  her  marital  problems  are  suggested  by  her 
Level  III-T  score.  Masochism  and  guilt  saturate  her  "preconscious" 
fantasies.  Her  smug,  righteous,  superior  fagade  pushed  her  husband 
to  bitter,  delinquent  rebellion  and  to  wild  retahating  rages.  The  pun- 


434        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

ishment  she  took  from  him  seemed  related  to  her  underlying  self- 
punitive,  guilty  feelings. 

We  are  particularly  interested  in  Patient  "P's"  relations  with  Pa- 
tient "M."  Figure  57  indicates  that  she  perceives  "A4"  as  being  re- 
sentful, complaining  and  rebellious.  She  consciously  equates  "M" 
with  her  husband. 

Consider  now  the  diagnostic  grid  for  Patient  "M."  She  sees  her- 
self (II)  as  a  docile,  weak,  self-punitive  person.  She  is  seen  by  the 
group  (I)  as  exhibiting  a  similar  slightly  more  masochistic  role.  Actu- 
ally this  summary  score  for  Level  I  is  the  resultant  of  two  somewhat 
different  perceptions  the  group  members  had  of  her.  Some  members 
saw  her  as  helpless,  weak  and  dependent.  Two  members  saw  "M"  as 
resentful  and  complaining.  We  recall  that  Patient  "P"  was  one  of 
these.  Patient  "M"  spent  most  of  her  time  in  the  group  describing 
her  failures  and  shortcomings — as  a  wife,  mother,  neighbor,  house- 
keeper, etc.  She  continually  blamed  herself  for  not  meeting  her  own 
inordinately  high  standards.  In  responding  to  other  group  members 
(except  "P"),  she  tended  to  be  a  silent  listener — never  advising  or 
attacking.  Her  relationship  to  "P,"  however,  was  quite  different.  She 
continually  nagged  "P"  to  present  her  problems,  accused  *'P"  of  being 
smug,  challenged  "P's"  bland  denial  of  problems.  Considerable  elec- 
tricity was  in  evidence  when  these  two  interacted.  Most  of  the  other 
members  were  admiring  or  abashed  or  helplessly  overwhelmed  by 
"P's"  superior  role — in  contrast  to  "M's"  irritated  needling  of  "P." 

This  relationship  is  objectified  in  "M's"  perception  of  "P"  in  Fig- 
ure 57.  She  describes  "P"  as  cold,  arrogant  and  narcissistic. 

Patient  "M"  sees  her  mother  in  the  same  way  as  she  describes  "P." 
As  her  case  history  unfolded  in  the  group  it  became  clear  that  "M" 
was  bitterly  entangled  with  her  mother.  She  described  her  mother  as 
a  bossy,  righteous  person  with  fanatic  standards  of  virtue  and  obedi- 
ence who  never  admitted  to  any  failure.  Her  mother's  punitive  cold- 
ness had  intimidated  "M"  for  many  years  and  she  was  still  loaded  with 
fear,  guilt  and  resentment  towards  her  mother. 

Patient  "M's"  Level  III  also  falls  in  the  narcissistic  sector  of  the 
circle.  This  indicates  that  she  was  "preconsciously"  identified  with 
her  mother's  strength  and  hostility. 

It  is  now  possible  to  fit  together  the  multilevel  of  the  two  patients 
and  to  observe  the  neat  dovetailing  of  narcissistic  and  masochistic  con- 
flicts. 

Patient  "P"  was  an  overt  narcissist  with  underlying  guilt  and 
masochism.  Her  overt  smugness  provoked  angry  rebellion  from  her 
husband  and  from  "M."  Her  original  question  which  led  to  her  ther- 
apy was:  "Why  does  my  husband  resent  me?"   It  was  answered  in 


PREDICTING  BEHAVIOR  IN  GROUP  THERAPY 


435 


part  by  her  effect  on  Patient  "M."  The  latter  expended  considerable 
energy  in  battering  away  at  "P's"  facade  and  eventually  stimulated 
and  led  a  revolt  of  the  group  against  "P."  A  second  answer  to  "P's" 
question  is  supplied  by  her  own  "preconscious"  feelings.  It  eventu- 
ally became  clear  that  "P"  had  deep  feelings  of  shame,  guilt,  and  in- 
feriority. She  had  remained  in  a  most  unhappy  marriage  for  over  two 
years,  supporting  her  husband,  accepting  his  beatings,  paying  his 
gambling  debts,  nursing  him  through  hangovers  because  of  her  un- 
derlying feeling  that  she  deserved  no  better. 

Patient  "M"  was  on  the  other  hand  an  overt  masochist  with  under- 
lying feelings  of  moral  superiority  and  narcissism.  She  had  always 
identified  herself  ("preconsciously")  with  her  mother's  cold,  harsh 
righteousness  and  overtly  suffered  in  order  to  maintain  the  inner 
commitment  to  these  standards.  The  original  question  which  led  her 
to  seek  treatment  was:  "Why  do  I  feel  depressed,  guilty,  and  weak?" 
This  was  answered  in  part  by  her  reaction  to  "P."  Patient  *'M,"  we 
recall,  was  the  first  group  member  to  spot  "P's"  superiority  and  re- 
luctance to  act  like  a  patient  with  problems.  Therapists  who  work 
from  the  interpersonal  viewpoint  are  always  alert  to  pick  up  and  focus 
on  relationships  of  this  sort.  It  became  clear  that  "M"  was  extremely 
sensitive  to  "P's"  behavior  because  most  of  her  energies  were  tied  up  in 
her  ambivalence  towards  her  mother.  A  second  answer  to  "M's"  ques- 
tion is  furnished  by  her  own  underlying  feelings — which  indicated 
an  identification  with  her  mother's  superior  standards. 

Patient  "P"  served  as  a  most  therapeutic  figure  for  "M."  "P" 
served  as  a  magnet  pulling  from  "M"  the  intensely  conflicted  feelings 
which  created  her  neurosis.  "M"  transferred  to  "P"  her  perception  of 
her  mother  and  relived  in  the  group  the  crucial,  embattled  relation- 
ship. 

Patient  "M"  was  an  extremely  valuable  figure  for  Patient  "P." 
"M's"  sensitive  radar  picked  up  "P's"  irritating,  patronizing  superior- 
ity and  made  it  clear  to  the  other  members  and  eventually  to  "P"  her- 
self. When  "M"  attacked  and  complained  about  "P,"  she  reproduced 
in  the  experimental  subsociety  of  the  group  the  problem  which  was 
destroying  "P's"  marriage  and  wrecking  her  life. 

This  paired  pattern  of  multilevel  projection,  misperception,  iden- 
tification, and  role-reciprocity  locked  these  two  patients  in  a  complex 
relationship.  The  analysis  of  this  relationship  reached  close  to  the 
roots  of  both  patients'  conflicts. 

Other  patients  in  this  group  became  entangled  in  multilevel  rela- 
tionships which  were  equally  complicated.  Another  naive,  ultracon- 
ventional  member  ("C")  became  engaged  in  an  intense  conflict  with 
a  rebellious  psychopath  ("S").  The  friction  between  these  two  pro- 


436        SOME  APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  INTERPERSONAL  SYSTEM 

vided  clues  and  an  observable  experimental  repetition  of  the  multi- 
level conformity-rebellion  problems  that  both  shared. 

In  these  illustrations  we  have  attempted  to  demonstrate  the  useful- 
ness of  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  system  in  understanding  multilevel 
reciprocal  relationships  as  they  occur  in  group  psychotherapy.  Two 
of  the  four  major  applications  of  the  interpersonal  system  have  been 
brought  to  bear  on  this  problem — its  use  as  a  multilevel  diagnostic 
instrument  and  its  use  as  a  tool  for  analyzing  group  dynamics.  The 
group  psychotherapy  situation  serves  as  a  unique  testing  ground  for 
the  rudimentary  techniques  of  the  present  and  as  a  developing  ground 
for  the  improved  techniques  which  we  know  must  follow. 

This  volume  is  concluded  with  no  sense  of  completion  or  closure, 
but  with  an  impatient  dissatisfaction  resulting  from  the  many  limita- 
tions of  these  shaky  initial  steps.  It  is  fitting  that  we  terminate — per- 
haps abruptly — in  a  discussion  of  the  complexity  of  multilevel  rela- 
tionships in  psychotherapy.  This  serves,  at  least,  to  point  the  course 
towards  the  two  problems  which  are  currently  engaging  the  energies 
of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  research  project:  a  study  of  the  interlevel 
mechanisms  of  personality  organization  and  the  measurement  of 
changes  in  personality  during  psychotherapy. 

Reference 

1.   Leary,   T.,   and   H.   S.   Coffey.    The   prediction    of    interpersonal   behavior   in 
group  psychotherapy.   Psychodrama  gp.  psychother.  Monogr.,  1955,  No.  28. 


Appendices 


Illustrations  of  the  Measurement  of 
Interpersonal  Behavior  at  Level  I 


This  chapter  presents  the  detailed  methodology  for  obtaining  four  dif- 
ferent Level  I  scores.  First  we  shall  illustrate  the  derivation  of  MMPI 
symptomatic  indices  (Level  I-M).  Next  we  shall  consider  the  Level 
I-P  indices  which  forecast  interpersonal  behavior  to  be  anticipated. 
Then  we  shall  follow  two  sample  patients  (a  typical  ulcer  patient  and 
a  severe  neurotic)  in  the  initial  minutes  of  a  recorded  group  therapy 
session.  This  will  illustrate  the  technique  for  obtaining  Level  I-R 
ratings.  This  will  be  followed  by  a  description  of  the  Level  I-S  indices 
for  one  of  these  patients  and  an  illustration  of  the  method  for  calcu- 
lating these  sociometric  indices. 

Illustration  of  the  Level  I-M  Predictive  Indices 

During  the  initial  diagnostic  and  evaluation  period  every  new  pa- 
tient reporting  to  the  Kaiser  Foundation  psychiatric  clinic  is  ad- 
ministered the  complete  interpersonal  test  battery,  including  the 
MMPI.  The  intake  conference  uses  all  the  available  clinical  informa- 
tion, in  addition  to  the  test  material,  to  make  recommendations  for 
type  of  treatment. 

In  this  section  we  shall  describe  a  method  for  making  interpersonal 
predictions  from  the  Minnesota  Multiphasic  Personality  Inventory.^ 
These  procedures  are  part  of  a  larger  study  in  interpersonal  diagnosis 
in  which  we  are  attempting  to  develop  MMPI  indices  which  predict 

'  The  Minnesota  Multiphasic  Personality  Inventory  comprises  nine  clinical,  psychi- 
atric scales  and  four  validating  scales.  These  are  entitled  as  follows:  Hs  =  hypo- 
chondriasis; D  =  depression;  Hy  =  hysteria;  Pd  zr  psychopathic  deviate;  Mf  refers 
to  masculinity-femininity  tendencies;  Pa  =  paranoia;  Pt  =  psychasthenia  or  obsessive 
tendencies;  Sc  =  schizoid  tendencies;  Ma  =  mania.  F  =  a  tendency  to  answer  items 
in  a  statistically  deviant  manner;  K  =  a  tendency  towards  a  defensive  denial  of  psy- 
chopathology;?  =  items  questioned  or  unanswered;  L  =  a  tendency  to  falsify  or  to 
answer  in  a  socially  acceptable  way. 

439 


^o  APPENDICES 

to  different  levels  of  interpersonal  behavior.  Other  MMPI  indices 
are  being  developed  which  predict  to  conscious,  "preconscious,"  and 
value  levels  of  personality. 

We  have  seen  in  Chapter  6  that  the  interpersonal  diagnostic  circle 
can  be  viewed  as  a  two-dimensional  surface  in  which  points  are  lo- 
cated in  reference  to  the  vertical  (i.e.,  dominance-submission)  and 
horizontal  (hostihty-affiliation)  axes.  The  interactions  of  any  subject 
can  be  converted  into  the  horizontal  and  vertical  tendencies,  thus  pro- 
viding a  single  summary  point. 

In  attempting  to  convert  the  MMPI  into  an  instrument  for  pre- 
dicting interpersonal  behavior,  the  same  procedure  was  followed. 
Two  years'  experience  in  comparing  MMPI  profiles  with  interpersonal 
profiles  provided  many  clinical  cues  as  to  the  relationship  between  the 
two.  These  cues  were  tested  in  a  series  of  pilot  studies  sortings  on 
several  hundred  cases  (1).  These  exploratory  procedures  suggested 
that  eight  of  the  MMPI  scales  were  related  to  the  role  behavior  of 
patients  seen  in  the  clinic.  Four  of  the  MMPI  scales — Ma,  D,  Hs,  and 
Pt — seem  to  be  correlated  with  dominant-submissive  behavior.  Four 
other  scales  seemed  to  be  related  to  friendly-hostile  behavior.  These 
are  Hy,  Sc,  K,  and  F.  The  absolute  height  of  these  scales  then  taken 
by  themselves  has  a  varying  prognostic  value;  some  have  low  correla- 
tions with  interpersonal  behavior;  others  are  surprisingly  high.  When 
the  general  pattern  of  their  interrelationship  was  studied,  significant 
predictions  resulted. 

The  first  set  of  indices  for  predicting  dominant  or  submissive  inter- 
personal behavior  are: 

Ma  —  D:  If  Ma  >  D,  a  -f-  score  results.  This  indicates  that  inter- 
personal strength,  assertion,  and  confidence  are  empha- 
sized. If  Ma  <  D,  the  opposite  is  indicated.  Weakness,  im- 
mobilization, and  lack  of  confidence  are  suggested. 

Hs  —  Pt:  If  Hs  >  Pt,  a  -f  score  results.  The  subject  seems  to  be  in- 
dicating that  his  physical  health  concerns  him  more  than 
emotional  worries.  This  is  the  wounded-warrior  theme 
often  expressed  by  psychosomatic  patients.  The  subject 
admits  to  some  bodily  weakness,  but  emotional  strength 
is  by  comparison  stronger.  If  Hs  <  Pt,  the  opposite  is 
true.  The  subject  is  more  concerned  with  his  emotional 
problems  and  is  emphasizing  fears,  worries,  or  immobili- 
zation. 

For  predicting  affiliative  or  hostile  behavior  four  MMPI  scales  are 
combined  as  follows: 

K  —  F:  If  K  >  F,  the  subject  tends  to  present  himself  as  a  helpful, 
friendly,  outgoing  person.  If  K  <  F,  the  patient  tends  to 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR 


441 


be  judged  as  alienated,  disaffiliatdve,  rebellious,  unfriendly. 
A  positive  score  on  this  index  thus  pulls  toward  the  right 
or  friendly  side  of  the  circle;  a  negative  score  in  the  hostile 
direction. 
Hy  —  Sc:  In  this  index  the  same  trends  appear.  High  Hy  scores 
correlated  with  bland,  naive,  superficially  agreeable  be- 
havior; high  Sc  scores  with  isolated  hostile  roles.  Thus  a 
positive  score  on  this  index  pulls  to  the  right  and  a  nega- 
tive score  to  the  left  of  the  circle. 

In  this  manner  eight  MMPI  scores  ^  can  be  converted  into  vertical 
and  horizontal  indices  and  translated  into  the  language  of  the  inter- 
personal system.  Four  scales — Ma,  Hs,  D,  and  Pt — when  pooled  yield 
a  vertical  (dominance-submission)  factor,  and  four  other  scales — K, 
Hy,  F,  and  Sc — yield  a  horizontal  (love-hate)  factor.  When  the 
vertical  and  horizontal  factors  are  plotted  on  the  two-dimensional  sur- 
face of  the  interpersonal  circle,  a  summary  point  is  obtained  which 
becomes  the  prediction  of  future  role  interactions.  Later  correlational 
studies,  which  are  presented  elsewhere  ( 1 ) ,  have  suggested  that  other 
combinations  of  MMPI  scores  may  yield  more  effective  predictions. 
Cross-validation  studies  on  the  new  formulas  have  not  been  accom- 
plished at  present.  The  current  findings  (clinical  and  correlational) 
have  demonstrated  that  the  eight  scales  described  above  with  certain 
qualifications  perform  adequately  in  assessing  the  interpersonal  mean- 
ing of  the  patient's  symptoms.  The  formulas  which  have  just  been 
presented  yield  scores  which  are  designated  Level  I-M. 

After  these  MMPI  symptomatic  indices  were  obtained,  the  next 
step  was  to  standardize  them.  The  standardization  sample  chosen  was 
the  entire  intake  population  of  a  psychiatric  clinic  over  a  two-year 
period.  The  787  cases  which  comprise  this  sample  may  be  divided  into 
two  clinical  groups  roughly  equal  in  size — those  referred  by  physi- 
cians for  psychosomatic  symptoms,  and  self-referrals.  The  MMPI 
was  routinely  administered  to  all  the  patients  who  were  evaluated  by 
intake  procedures. 

The  two  MMPI  indices  (horizontal  and  vertical)  were  standardized 
so  that  the  indices  for  each  patient  can  be  expressed  in  terms  of  their 
distance  from  the  mean  of  the  total  sample. 

It  is  thus  possible  to  plot  each  patient's  Level  I-M  scores  on  the 
interpersonal  diagnostic  grid  and  to  indicate  the  intensity  and  type  of 
the  predicted  behavior.  The  center  of  the  circle  was  determined  by 
the  means  of  the  horizontal  and  vertical  distributions.  The  distance 
and  direction  from  the  center  of  the  circle  automatically  "types"  the 
Level  I  symptomatic  behavior  in  terms  of  the  sixteen  variables. 

^  In  all  of  the  MMPI  indices  the  K-corrected  standard  scores  are  employed. 


442 


APPENDICES 


By  way  of  illustration,  let  us  consider  two  MMPI  profiles  of  male 
patients.  The  solid  line  in  Figure  58  indicates  the  MMPI  pattern  of  a 
patient  who  came  to  the  clinic  complaining  of  neurotic  symptoms — 
immobilization,  depression,  and  marital  discord.  The  dotted  line  in- 
dicates the  record  of  an  ulcer  patient  referred  by  his  physician  for 
psychological  evaluation.  Table  38  shows  interpersonal  conversions 
of  the  MMPI  profiles.  The  standard  scores  used  in  the  formulae  are 
located  on  the  extreme  left  and  right  hand  sides  of  the  profile  sheet, 
labeled  "T  or  Tc." 


H3t.5K        D  Hy     Pdt  4K       Ml  Pa       Pi  tlK    Sc41KMa<.2K       Si       ToiTc 


Hy      Pd+.4K      Mf 


Figure  58.    MMPI  Profiles  on  a  "Classic  Neurotic"  Patient  (Solid  Line)  and  an 
Ulcer  Patient  (Dotted  Line). 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR  443 

TABLE  38 

Illustrative  Calculation  of  MMPI  Indices  for 
Measuring  Symptomatic  Behavior  (Level  I-M) 

MMPI  Index                                   "Neurotic"  Patient  (Male)  Ulcer  Patient  (Male) 

Ma-D     58 -84  =  -26  58 -46  =  +12 

Hs-Pt     59 -75  =  -16  82 -66  =  +16 

Vertical  total —42  +28 

Vertical  total 

converted  to  standard  score 43  72 

K-F    44 -70  =  -32  70  -  53  = +17 

Hy-Sc    65 -84  =  -19  67  -  63  =  +  4 

Horizontal  total   —51  +21 

Horizontal  total 

converted  to  standard  score     ...  33  60 

The  horizontal  and  vertical  scores  for  these  two  patients  were  then 
plotted  on  the  standardized  diagnostic  grid.^  (See  Figure  59)  The 
resulting  summary  points  indicate  that  entirely  different  interpersonal 
behavior  can  be  expected  from  these  two  patients.  The  neurotic's 
MMPI  predictive  indices  for  Level  I  place  him  in  the  octant  FG.  To 
the  extent  that  the  MMPI  conversions  hold  good,  we  should  anticipate 
sullen,  rebellious,  and  passively  hostile  behavior.  The  ulcer  patient 
falls  into  the  octant  NO — thus  we  can  expect  that  responsible,  strong, 
and  helpful  behavior  will  develop.  The  neurotic  patient  will  com- 
plain of  symptoms  in  a  passively  coercive  manner.  The  ulcer  patient 
will  probably  manifest  no  overt  hostile,  dependent,  or  weak  behavior. 

These  symptomatic  indices  have  become  the  standard  Level  I  diag- 
nostic tool  for  the  psychiatric  clinic.  At  the  time  of  intake  evaluation, 
the  symptomatic  pressure  exerted  by  the  patient  is  of  crucial  impor- 
tance in  planning  a  therapeutic  program.  All  of  the  diagnostic  studies 
reported  in  Chapters  15-22  employ  Level  I-M  measures. 

These  indices  do  not  work  as  well  in  other  functional  settings. 
They  would  not  be  used,  for  example,  to  predict  behavior  in  an  in- 
dustrial office  or  in  group  discussion  situations.  The  subject's  emo- 
tional symptoms  are  not  the  crucial  factors  determining  behavior  in 
these  environments. 

When  Level  I-M  indices  are  correlated  with  sociometric  ratings  of 
group  behavior  (Level  I-S),  the  relationships,  while  significantly  posi- 
tive, are  not  high  enough  for  use  in  predicting  roles  in  group  psycho- 

^The  standard  score  conversions  of  Level  I-M  which  are  employed  in  the  diag- 
nostic grid  of  Figure  58  are  based  on  a  sample  of  787  psychiatric  clinic  patients.  The 
mean  of  the  vertical  distribution  (i.e..  Ma  —  D  +  Hs  —  Pt)  is  —24.4,  and  the  sigma  is 
24.1.  The  mean  of  the  horizontal  distribution  (i.e.,  K  — F  +  Hy  — Sc)  is  —6.13,  and 
the  sigma  is  27.1.  A  table  for  converting  Level  I-M  dominance  and  hostility  indices 
into  standard  scores  is  presented  in  Appendix  5. 


444 


APPENDICES 


^f^«4S0CHISTlC 

Figure  59.  Diagnostic  Grid  for  Locating  Level  I-M  Diagnosis.  Key:  The  center  of 
the  circle  represents  the  mean  score  of  the  horizontal  and  vertical  distribution  (stand- 
ard score  of  50).  Each  cahbrated  line  on  the  grid  equals  one  standard  deviation.  To 
locate  the  predicted  interpersonal  role  on  the  circular  grid:  (1)  Determine  the  hori- 
zontal and  veracal  indices,  from  the  MMPI  formulas.  (2)  Locate  the  horizontal  co- 
ordinate and  the  vertical  coordinate  on  the  major  axes  of  the  diagnostic  circle.  The 
major  axes  are  calibrated  so  as  to  convert  the  raw  MMPI  indices  into  standard  scores. 
(3)  The  posiuon  where  these  intersect  determines  the  predicted  interpersonal  role. 
The  farther  from  the  center,  the  more  extreme  and  maladjustive  the  symptomatic 
pressure  exerted  by  the  subject.  The  two  illustrative  cases  whose  indices  were  calcu- 
lated in  Table  38  are  plotted  on  this  grid.  Ulcer  patient  =  U,  and  the  patient  with 
classic  neurotic  symptoms  =  N. 

therapy.  This  means  that  Level  I-M  indices  which  work  well  at  the 
intake  diagnostic  level  are  less  useful  in  predicting  how  the  patient 
will  behave  in  group  therapy. 

Level  I-P  Indices  for  Predicting  Behavior  in  Group  Therapy 

The  limitations  of  the  Level  I-M  indices  made  it  necessary  to  de^ 
velop  improved  methods  for  predicting  behavior  in  group  therapy. 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR  445 

Two  criterion-specific  MMPI  scales  (one  for  dominance  and  one 
for  love)  have  been  developed  which  predict  role  behavior  in  groups. 
These  provide  the  Level  I-P  diagnosis.  Each  of  the  550  MMPI  items 
was  studied  to  determine  its  relationship  to  I-S  dominance-submission 
and  love-hate.  The  items  which  discriminated  the  Level  I-S  vertical 
factor  (at  a  level  of  statistical  significance)  were  combined  into  a  Level 
I-P  index  for  dominance.  A  scale  was  also  constructed  which  relates 
to  the  horizontal  (hostility)  factor.  These  scales  are  in  the  process  of 
cross-validation. 

Illustration  of  the  Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Reflexes  from 
Group  Therapy  Interactions 

The  most  straightforward  method  of  coding  interpersonal  reflexes 
is  to  rate  the  blow-by-blow  interaction  sequence — either  observed 
directly  or  followed  on  electric  recordings  or  typed  transcripts. 

The  two  sample  patients  (ulcer  and  severe  neurotic)  whose  pre- 
dictive indices  were  calculated  in  the  previous  section  were  assigned 
to  the  same  therapy  group. 

We  shall  now  present  a  transcription  of  the  first  few  minutes  of 
the  initial  group  therapy  session.  The  severe  neurotic  patient  de- 
scribed above  is  coded  "SN"  and  the  classic  ulcer  patient,  "ULC." 

In  this  passage  the  interpersonal  reflexes  are  scored  at  the  right. 
The  scoring  of  each  mechanism  consists  of  three  ratings:  the  code  let- 
ter representing  the  location  of  the  action  along  the  circular  con- 
tinuum of  interpersonal  mechanisms,  the  verb  conside»-ed  most  closely 
descriptive  of  the  action,  and  the  rating  of  intensity  of  the  mechanism 
along  the  four-point  scale.  In  practice,  the  scoring  of  the  descriptive 
verb  may  be  omitted. 

GROUP   THERAPY   PROTOCOL 

Six  male  patients  file  into  a  room  and  seat  themselves  expectantly.  One  pa- 
tient, SN,  glances  at  a  picture  on  the  wall  of  the  therapist's  office  and  begins 
the  group  therapy  process  by  remarking: 

Mechanism  or 

Reflex  Code 

1  SN  Is  that  suppose  to  be  art  on  the  wall.      Ridicules  D-3 

or  is  that  something  somebody  drew 
in  the  hospital? 

2  Th  Now,  the  purpose  of  our  meeting  in      Teaches  P-2 

general  is  to  help  each  of  you  to  come 
to  a  better  understanding  of  yourself, 
a  deeper  understanding  of  yourself. 
The  meetings  will  last  about  an  hour 
and  a  half,  we'll  meet  for  at  least  four 


446 


3    Th 


4    SN 


5    Th 


6    SN 


7    EE 


8    AA 


9    SN 


10    Th 


11    EE 


12    Th 


months,  at  least  15  or  16  times.  [Ther- 
apist continues  structuring  for  two 
minutes.] 

.  .  .  I'm  going  to  throw  the  burden  of 
the  conversation  now  to  the  group. 
I'd  like  to  have  you  tell  yourself  and 
tell  us,  today  and  for  the  next  few 
days,  who  you  are,  why  you  see  your- 
self coming  here  and  what  you  might 
want  to  get  from  the  group. 

What  if  you  don't  know?  What  if 
you  haven't  the  slightest  idea  what 
you  want? 

Well,  that's  a  good  place  to  start.  You 
have  already  told  us  something  inter- 
esting about  yourself. 

How  can  you  talk  about  something 
you  don't  know  anything  about? 

We  could  have  a  sympathizers'  club 
here. 

A  friend  of  mine  suggested  that,  as  a 
matter  of  fact. 

Is  that  it?  Are  we  supposed  to  cry  on 
each  other's  shoulders?  Is  that  the  ob- 
ject of  it?  Crocodile  tears  and  sympa- 
thetic ears,  is  that  the  idea? 

(smiles)  I  hope  we  can  help  each 
other  more  than  just  by  groaning  to- 
gether. 

Mr.  SN  has  said  ...  I  felt  that  way 
too  about  knowing  what  I  want,  but  I 
think  that's  more  a  problem  of  just 
...?...  or  you've  just  gotten  so 
darn  discouraged  about  things  you 
just  don't  .  .  .  well,  there's  a  feeling, 
I  know  with  myself  .  .  .  feeling  that 
what  a  lot  of  people  want  just  aren't 
worth  a  candle,  that's  all  .  .  .  it  seems 
to  take  too  much  out  of  me  in  the  way 
of  effort  and  emotional  drive  or  some- 
thing. 

You  don't  just  want  the  things  that 
any  other  people  want? 


APPENDICES 

Mechanism  or 

Reflex  Code 


Directs 


Supports 


Summarizes 


A-l 


Passively  resists       f-3 


N-2 


Passively  resists      F-l 


Ridicules  self 
and  others 

H-l 
D-1 

Agrees 

L-l 

Passively  resists 

f-3 

Ridicules 

D-2 

1-2 

Takes  weak 
position 

P-2 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR 


447 


15     EE  Yes,  about  a  lot  of  things  .  .  .  like,  for 

example,  a  good  deal  of  my  problems 
center  about  my  work.  I  kind  of  ra- 
tionalize that  by  saying,  "Oh,  to  hell 
with  it!  Most  things  people  do  aren't 
worth  doing  anyway."  I  repair  air 
conditioners  for  a  living,  for  example. 
I  have  a  very  bad  attitude  about  the 
sets.  Firstly,  I  can't  sympathize  with 
my  customers.  I  don't  see  why  they 
want  to  keep  the  damn  things  going, 
(laughs)  And,  they  hound  me  to  get 
the  work.  Of  course,  with  my  atti- 
tude, one  could  make  a  virtue  of  it. 
You  could  say  that  I  have  infinite  pa- 
tience. Frankly,  I  don't  care.  I'm  not 
anxious  to  get  paid,  I'm  not  anxious  to 
get  started,  I'm  not  anxious  to  finish. 
In  fact,  I  feel  a  good  deal  of  anxiety 
frequently  about  getting  started  and  I 
think  that's  part  of  Mr.  SN's  .  .  .  that 
there's    something    there— a    counter 


Mechanism  or 

Reflex 
Condemns  self 


13  EE  Yeah,  I  seem  to  have  gotten  into  an 

attitude  of  what  you  might  call  emo- 
tional dumbness  where  I  don't  just 
seem  to  have  the  emotional  level  that 
some  people  have.  Some  people  get 
enthused  about  going  to  a  picnic,  ball 
game  or  this,  that  and  the  other  thing 
...  I  mean,  speaking  for  myself,  I'll 
say,  "Ah,  just  let  me  alone."  If  some- 
body's going  to  a  picnic,  I  don't  give 
a  damn  whether  they  go  to  Milpitas, 
San  Francisco  or  what  not.  I  don't  ex- 
pect to  enjoy  myself  at  a  picnic.  I'd 
rather  stay  home  and  sit  on  my  butt 
and  thumb  through  a  magazine  or 
something  .  .  .  keep  comfortable  and 
not  bother  with  anything. 

14  ULC       You're   speaking  generally   now— not      Explains 

just  about  a  picnic?  About  many 
things  .  .  .  Because  I  was  going  to 
say,  there  are  a  lot  of  people  who  take 
that  attitude  about  certain  things.  You 
can  never  get  them  to  work  up  en- 
thusiasms to  get  them  to  do  anything, 
and  yet  they  have  other  outlets,  or  are 
enthusiastic  about  .  .  . 


Code 
H-3 


P-3 


Depreciates  self      H-} 


Pulls  for 
sympathy 


;-3 


448 


16    EE 


17    Th 


18    EE 


19    Th 


20  EE 

21  Th 


22    SN 


23    Th 


24    SN 


That's  right. 

Now  let's  stop  for  a  moment.  Have 
these  themes  made  anything  click  as 
you've  listened  to  EE  describe  them? 

The  idea  of  the  annoyance  of  being 
pressed  is  common.  That's  common 
to  everybody,  isn't  it?  When  you  get 
somebody  on  your  tail  and  you  know 
that  they  are  right  and  they  have  justi- 
fication in  their  claims  and  that  you 
can't  satisfy  them  or  .  .  .  and  then 
you  feel  a  negative  attitude  .  .  .  you 
would  like  to  take  their  work  and 
throw  it  out. 

What  do  you  usually  do  when  you 
feel  that  ...  do  you  throw  their 
work  out? 

No,  you  just  smile,  and  say,  "Oh, 
that's  too  bad." 


APPENDICES 

Mechanism  or 

Reflex  Code 


Accuses,  de- 
scribes self 
as  exploited 


force.  It  isn't  that  a  person  doesn't 
know  what  they  want  to  do,  really.  1 
mean  that  there's  some  counter  force 
that  makes  anything  that  you  want  to 
do  not  worth  the  price.  (He  con- 
tinues at  length  in  this  vein.) 

.  .  .  they  feel  that  I  don't  have  a  damn 
bit  of  interest  in  their  particular  prob- 
lems and  even  though  my  proposition 
is  reasonable  ...  in  many  cases  I've 
gotten  turned  down.  I  had  an  example 
of  that  recently.  By  the  way,  am  I 
taking  up  too  much  of  the  time? 

Let's  stop  a  minute  because  you  have 
raised  several  interesting  themes,  the 
feeling  of  obligation,  the  feeling  of 
being  pressed  in  on  by  forces  that  .  .  . 

Like  when  I  rest  .  .  .  like  last  night 
when  my  wife  says  "Well,  do  you 
want  to  go  out  tonight?"  I  have  al- 
ready complained  about  being  tired 
and  I  did  feel  tired.  It  was  about 
eight-thirty  that  I  started  out. 

Somewhat  against  your  will,  but  you      Reflects 
did  go. 


Directs 
Summarizes 


Therapeutic 
question 


G-3 


Mildly  criticizes     H-1 
self 


A-2 
P-1 


Accuses  others       G-3 
Passively  resists       F-3 
therapist's 
direction 


0-2 


Participates  L-\ 

Directs  A-2 


Gives  opinion         P-2 


0-2 


Depreciates  self      ^-2 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR 


449 


25  Th  Is  that  what  you  usually  do? 

26  SN  Well,  sometimes  I  sort  of  digress  a  lit- 

tle bit  and  I  carry  on  a  little  campaign 
trying  to  impress  people  that  there  is  a 
lot  more  to  it  .  .  .  that  they  are  ex- 
pecting more.  .  .  . 

27  Th  But  you've  never  been  in  that  position. 

28  SN  Never  been  in  that  position.    Some- 

times I've  wanted  things  and  the  next 
day  I  got  them  and  it  seems  as  though 
when  I  got  them  it  wasn't  what  I 
wanted  after  all. 

29  Th  What  have  your  thoughts  been  as  you 

have  listened? 

30  ULC       Well,  first  of  all,  Mr.  SN's  statement 

of  enthusiasm,  followed  by  a  period  of 
less  enthusiasm  or  depression,  accord- 
ing to  my  understanding  is  more  or 
less  normal  to  a  certain  degree,  now  if 
that  goes  to  a  greater  degree,  maybe 
that's  not  normal.  All  of  us  have  per- 
iods where  we  work  easily  and  enjoy 
our  work.  Now  whether  it  is  during 
that  period  you  also  suffer  some  of 
that  anxiety  you  said  you  worried 
about  the  periods  that  are  coming.  .  .  . 

3 1  Th  How  do  you  compare  with  SN  or  EE 

as  they  have  presented  their  situation? 

32  ULC       Well,  it  didn't  quite  fit  in  exactly.  My 

work  is  a  little  different  and  I  don't 
have  to  meet  the  public.  I'm  a  .  .  . 
and  as  such  I  work  under  the  director- 
ship of  the  department  head,  the  group 
leader,  so  that,  while  we  have  pressure 
on  us  at  times  to  do  work,  it  isn't  the 
idea  you're  worrying  about  the  busi- 
ness ahead,  or  discouraging  customers. 

33  BB  Do  you  worry  about  your  work  com- 

ing out  right?  Does  that  give  you 
anxiety  feelings? 

34  ULC       I  think  ...  is  very  frustrating  in  one 

respect  and  that  is  that  it  seems  like 
ninety  percent  of  the  time  or  greater 


Mechanis?n  or 

Reflex 
Therapeutic 
question 

Mildly  praises 
self 


Summarizes 

Feels 
disappointed 


Calls 


Pedantically 
teaches 


Code 
0-2 


B-l 


P-2 
G-2 


A-2 


P-3 


Therapeutic  0-2 

question 

Denies  problem      B-2 


Therapeutic 
question 

Describes 
frustration 


0-2 


G-2 


450 


APPENDICES 


Mechanism  or 

Reflex  Code 


your  .  .  .  what  you  do  does  not  come 
out  in  a  favorable  manner.  In  other 
words  you  are  only  looking  for  the 
few  successful  experiments.  That's 
what  makes  the  money  for  the  com- 
pany. You  have  volumes  and  volumes 
of  papers  describing  work  you  did 
that  no  one  will  ever  look  at  again. 

35  BB  Does  that  worry  you— your  relation- 

ship with  your  immediate  superiors? 
Do  you  feel  that  maybe  you  haven't 
done  things  right  or  fast  enough  or 
careful  enough? 

36  ULC       Yes,  you  do  have  those  feelings  too, 

that's  true. 

37  EE  Well,  I  think  there's  a  sort  of  tie  in 

that  basically  it  is  simply  probably  you 
don't  meet  the  general  public  so  much 
as  that  your  problems  center  maybe 
on  one  or  two  individuals. 

38  Th  Have  you  had  this  feeling  of  pressure 

that  SN  or  EE  have  described? 

39  ULC       No,  not  too  much.  No. 


Therapeutic  0-2 

question 


Accepts  L-2 

Gives  opinion         P-2 


Therapeutic  0-2 

question 

Denies  problem       B-2 


This  passage  serves  as  a  nice  illustration  of  the  development  of  in- 
terpersonal reflex  patterns.  The  opening  moments  of  a  psychotherapy 
group  are  always  most  dramatic  and  important.  Six  strangers  come 
together,  meet  for  the  first  time,  and  begin  automatically  to  train  each 
other.  The  network  of  interaction,  perception  and  misperception  be- 
gins to  weave  itself.  Consider  Patient  "SN"  in  the  above  passage.  In 
the  first  five  seconds  of  the  group  he  has  launched  a  critical  and  skep- 
tical arrow.  He  challenges  the  therapist  sarcastically,  asking  about  a 
Picasso  print  on  the  wall.  We  may  suspect  that  through  these  com- 
ments he  is  telHng  not  just  the  therapist  but  the  group  in  general,  "I'm 
a  negative,  uncooperative  person;  you're  going  to  have  trouble  with 
me."  We  have  no  record  of  what  the  five  other  patients  were  doing 
while  Patient  "SN"  was  making  his  opening  gambit.  A  motion  pic- 
ture record  might  have  revealed  that  the  others  were,  in  their  own 
way,  beginning  to  develop  their  roles.  Patient  "ULC"  who  later  ex- 
presses himself  verbally  as  a  self-satisfied,  executive  person  might  very 
well  have  been  using  non-verbal  means  to  communicate  his  detached 
competence — crossing  his  legs  briskly   and  shooting   alert  glances 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR  451 

around  the  room.  Patient  "EE,"  who  is  soon  to  begin  building  a 
facade  of  self-critical  weakness  may,  in  these  opening  seconds,  have 
been  sending  sheepish,  apprehensive  glances  towards  the  others. 

As  we  follow  the  subsequent  moves  of  the  grumpy  Patient  "SN" 
we  see  the  same  reflex  pattern  unfolding  quite  consistently.  Interac- 
tions #6  and  #9  continue  to  communicate  the  theme  of  uncoopera- 
tive and  passive  resistance. 

As  Patient  "EE"  enters  the  action  (remarks  #11  and  #13)  a  dif- 
ferent set  of  reflexes  appear.  Patient  "EE"  begins  a  sequence  of  pas- 
sive self-effacement.  His  self-depreciatory  remarks  are  continued  at 
length  in  #15  and  we  sense  that  by  #16  that  they  have  developed 
into  a  repetitious  circle  of  pessimistic  ruminations.  He  apologizes  for 
monopolizing  the  discussion.  Interactions  #17  and  #18  focus  on  a 
most  interesting  transaction.  The  therapist  (#17)  attempts  to  check 
the  flow  of  anxiety-driven  words,  but  "EE"  (in  #18)  continues  his 
reflex  laments.  In  ignoring  the  therapist's  intervention,  "EE"  provides 
us  with  a  nice  illustration  of  the  involuntary  nature  of  Level  I  com- 
munication. We  may  safely  guess  that  this  patient  did  not  deliberately 
or  consciously  interrupt  and  disregard  the  therapist.  He  has  just  ex- 
pressed conscious  anxiety  about  talking  too  much,  but  automatically 
goes  on  to  produce  a  rather  flagrant  example  of  insensitive,  anxiety- 
driven  complaint. 

At  this  point,  it  will  be  seen  that  Mr.  "EE"  has  engaged  in  seven 
interactions  (#'s  7,  11,  13,  15,  16,  18,  and  20).  What  impression  can 
we  surmise  he  has  made  on  his  fellow  group  members?  On  the  thera- 
pist? These  sLx  communications  provide  the  data  for  a  small  experi- 
ment in  interpersonal  relations  in  which  the  reader  may  participate. 
Glance  back  over  Mr.  "EE's"  statements,  imagining  that  you  are  a 
member  of  this  therapy  group.  What  feelings  do  you  sense  in  re- 
sponse to  his  comments?  Some  readers  have  reported  a  feeling  of 
sympathy,  mixed  with  superiority,  and  irritable  impatience.  To  the 
extent  that  these  feelings  have  been  aroused  in  the  reader  then  to  that 
extent  "EE"  has  in  seven  easy  steps  taught  or  trained  the  reader  to 
respond  to  him  in  a  typical  and  consistent  way.  Mr.  "EE"  had  an 
unusually  rigid  and  inappropriate  set  of  reflexes — apologetic,  self- 
critical,  and  complaining.  He  trained  the  group  members  and  the 
therapist  just  as  he  had  trained  everyone  in  his  life  to  respond  to  him 
with  tolerant  and/or  irritable  superiority. 

This  set  of  reflex  responses  seemed  to  operate  as  a  defensive  ma- 
neuver. Occasionally  he  was  able  to  show  other  responses.  But  the 
more  anxious  he  became,  the  less  able  he  was  to  respond  appropriately 
and  the  more  driven  he  was  to  continue  his  interpersonal  defenses  (as 
illustrated  clearly  in  the  sequence  #17  and  #18). 


452 


APPENDICES 


It  is  possible  to  summarize  the  interactions  during  any  given  time 
period  in  a  diagram.  We  simply  count  up  the  interactions  for  each 
subject  and  chart  them  on  the  diagnostic  circle.  The  intensity  of  the 
interaction  can  be  graphically  illustrated  by  using  different  colors  in 
the  diagram  (green  for  intensity  1,  black  for  intensity  2,  etc.),  or  by 
multiplying  each  score  by  the  intensity  (so  that  one  F-3  would  equal 
three  F-l's).  Using  the  latter  technique,  we  have  included  a  diagram- 
matic summary  of  the  interactions  of  three  of  these  patients  in 
Figure  60. 

During  the  first  five  minutes  of  this  session,  we  see  that  Patient 
"SN"  is  rated  as  skeptical  and  passively  resistant;  Patient  "EE"  as 


«^h77 


Figure  60.  Summary  of  Interpersonal  Behavior  of  Three  Parients  During  First  Five 
Minutes  of  Group  Psychotherapy. 


MEASUREMENT  OF  LEVEL  I  BEHAVIOR  453 

weak,  self-derogatory,  and  passively  resistant;  while  Patient  "ULC" 
is  rated  as  executive,  strong,  and  self-satisfied. 

Illustration  of  the  Level  IS  Index  of  Interpersonal  Behavior 

The  preceding  sections  have  followed  two  sample  patients  through 
two  different  measurement  processes.  The  Level  I-M  scores  sum- 
marize the  interpersonal  meaning  of  their  symptoms.  The  Level 
I-R  scores  summarize  how  they  actually  did  behave  in  the  first  min- 
utes of  therapy.  We  shall  now  describe  another  estimate  of  inter- 
personal role  based  on  sociometric  ratings. 

After  six  sessions  of  therapy,  each  patient  in  the  group  was  ad- 
ministered the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List.  Each  subject  rated 
his  impression  of  every  other  patient  in  the  group  and  was  in  the  same 
way  rated  by  all  the  members  of  the  group. 

The  total  number  of  items  for  each  octant  attributed  to  each  mem- 
ber of  the  group  by  every  other  member  was  then  tabulated.  Table 
39  presents  these  data  for  the  severe  neurotic  patient  whom  we  have 
followed  in  the  previous  discussions. 

These  figures  have  many  uses.  It  is  possible  to  diagram  separately 
(either  in  raw  octant  totals  or  by  means  of  trigonometric  indices)  the 
perception  which  each  group  patient  has  about  Patient  "SN."  Inter- 
esting patterns  of  misperception  become  obvious.  In  this  case  it  is 
possible  to  determine  (from  Table  39)  that  Patient  "ULC"  views 
Patient  "SN"  differently  from  the  consensual  perception  of  the  group. 
He  attributes  more  docility  (JK)  and  tenderness  (NO)  to  Patient 
"SN." 


TABLE  39 

Illustration 

OF    • 

FHE  Calculations  for 

DETERMINfNG 

THE  Level  I  Profile  for  a 

"Neurotic" 

Patient,  SN 

Fellow  Group  Member  Making  Ratings 
of  Patient  SN 

Raw  Total  of  Words 

Assigned  b\  OroiH) 

Octant 

AA 

BB 

ULC 

EE 

FF 

toSN 

AP 

4 

4 

3 

2 

2 

15 

BC 

3 

2 

1 

1 

4 

11 

DE 

10 

4 

2 

4 

10 

30 

FG 

13 

10 

4 

8 

IS 

50 

HI 

14 

8 

13 

11 

14 

61 

JK 

10 

4 

10 

4 

7 

35 

LM 

4 

1 

6 

2 

3 

16 

NO 

3 

0 

11 

4 

0 

18 

Total  No. 

61 

33 

50 

36 

55 

235 

When  the  raw  octant  totals  are  placed  in  the  trigonometric  formulas:  Dom  = 
— 85.2;  Lov  =  — 19.6.  When  these  indices  are  divided  by  the  total  number  of  words 
(235):  Dom  =  -.363;  Lov  = -.083. 


454  APPENDICES 

At  this  point  we  are  concerned  with  the  derivation  of  the  summary- 
Level  I-S  score.  This  is  obtained  from  the  calculations  in  the  right- 
hand  column  of  Table  39.  Vertical  and  horizontal  indices  were  calcu- 
lated using  the  total  number  of  words  per  octant  assigned  by  the  group 
to  Patient  "SN." 

These  horizontal  and  vertical  indices  are  converted  into  standard 
scores  by  means  of  norms  derived  from  large  samples  of  group  ther- 
apy patients.  The  horizontal  and  vertical  standard  scores  become  the 
Level  I-S  indices. 

These  can  then  be  plotted  on  a  diagnostic  grid  to  determine  the 
Level  L-S  diagnosis.  The  summary  point  for  Patient  "SN"  locates 
in  the  extreme  sector  of  the  FG  octant.  The  Level  I-S  diagnosis  based 
on  a  consensual  sociometric  impression  of  fellow  group  members  thus 
duplicates  the  Level  I-R  score. 

Reference 

1.   Leary,  T.,  and  H.  Coffey.    The  prediction  of  interpersonal  behavior  in  group 
psychotherapy.   Psychodr.  and  gp.  psychother.  Monogr.,  1955,  No.  28. 


The  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List 


The  instrument  routinely  employed  to  measure  interpersonal  behavior 
at  Level  II-C,  Level  V-C,  and  Level  I-S  is  the  Interpersonal  Adjective 
Check  List.  This  measuring  device  has  been  designed  by  Robert 
Suczek,  Ph.D.,  Rolfe  LaForge,  Ph.D.,  and  the  other  members  of  the 
Kaiser  Foundation  psychology  staff  ( 1 ) .  It  has  been  subjected  to  in- 
tensive empirical  study  over  a  period  of  five  years  and  has  gone 
through  three  major  revisions. 

The  present  form  of  the  check  list  (Form  IV)  comprises  128  items 
— eight  for  each  of  the  sixteen  interpersonal  variables.  An  intensity 
dimension  has  been  built  into  the  check  list  such  that  each  of  the  six- 
teen variables  is  represented  by  a  four-point  scale.  For  each  variable 
there  is  one  intensity  /  item  which  reflects  "a  mild  or  necessary 
amount  of  the  trait."  Three  items  refer  to  intensity  2,  "a  moderate  or 
appropriate  amount  of  the  trait."  Three  words  reflect  intensity  3,  "a 
marked  or  inappropriate  amount  of  the  trait."  And  one  word  ex- 
presses intensity  4,  an  "extreme  amount  of  the  trait."  The  Form  IV 
check  list  arranged  by  variable  and  intensity  is  presented  in  Table  40. 
The  checklist  items  are  arranged  in  circular  form  in  Figure  6,  page  135. 

In  actual  use,  the  check  list  is  given  in  approximate  alphabetical 
order  and  the  subject  indicates  on  an  IBM  answer  sheet  the  items 
which  are  descriptive  of  himself  or  of  the  person  he  is  rating.  The 
check  list  has  also  been  administered  by  means  of  IBM  cards.  Each 
item  was  printed  on  a  separate  card  and  the  subject  was  requested  to 
sort  the  cards  into  "true"  and  "false"  piles.  These  cards  were  then 
machine-punched  and  provide  a  card  file  for  IBM  research  studies. 
The  methodology  for  IBM  administration  and  analysis  of  the  check 
list  is  described  by  LaForge  in  a  separate  paper  (2). 

*  This  chapter  is  a  summary  of  a  paper  written  by  Rolfe  LaForge,  Ph.D.,  and 
Robert  Suczek,  Ph.D.,  the  designers  of  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List.  Grati- 
tude is  expressed  to  Drs.  LaForge  and  Suczek  for  permitting  the  extended  quotation 
from  their  manuscript.  Table  and  footnote  numbers  of  the  original  paper  have  been 
altered  to  conform  to  the  series  of  such  numbers  in  the  present  volume. 

455 


456 


APPENDICES 


TABLE  40 

Interpersonal  Check  List,  Form  4, 
Words  Arranged  by  Octant  and  Intensity 


Octant  1:  A? 
A:  1  Able  to  give  orders 

2  Forceful 
Good  leader 
Likes  responsibility 

3  Bossy 
Dominating 
Manages  others 

4  Dictatorial 

Octant  2:  BC 
B:  1  Self-respecting 

2  Independent 
Self-confident 
Self-reliant  and  assertive 

3  Boastful 

Proud  and  self-satisfied 
Somewhat  snobbish 

4  Egotistical  and  conceited 

Octant  3:  DE 
D:  1  Can  be  strict  if  necessary 

2  Firm  but  just 
Hardboiled  when  necessary 
Stern  but  fair 

3  Impatient  with  others'  mistakes 
Self-seeking 

Sarcastic 

4  Cruel  and  unkind 

Octant  4:  FG 
F:  I  Can  complain  if  necessary 

2  Often  gloomy 
Resents  being  bossed 
Skeptical 

3  Bitter 
Complaining 
Resentful 

4  Rebels  against  everything 

Octant  $:  HI 
H:  1  Able  to  criticize  self 

2  Apologetic 
Easily  embarrassed 
Lacks  self-confidence 

3  Self-punishing 
Shy 

Timid 

4  Always  ashamed  of  self 


P:  1  Well  thought  of 

2  Makes  a  good  impression 
Often  admired 
Respected  by  others 

3  Always  giving  advice 
Acts  important 

Tries  to  be  too  successful 

4  Expects  everyone  to  admire  him 

C:  1  Able  to  take  care  of  self 

2  Can  be  indifferent  to  others 
Businesslike 

Likes  to  compete  with  others 

3  Thinks  only  of  himself 
Shrewd  and  calculating 
Selfish 

4  Cold  and  unfeeling 

E:  1  Can  be  frank  and  honest 

2  Critical  of  others 
Irritable 
Straightforward  and  direct 

3  Outspoken 
Often  unfriendly 
Frequently  angry 

4  Hard-hearted 


G:  1  Able  to  doubt  others 

2  Frequently  disappointed 
Hard  to  impress 
Touchy  and  easily  hun 

3  Jealous 

Slow  to  forgive  a  wrong 
Stubborn 


4  Distrusts  everybody 


/;  I  Can  be  obedient 

2  Usually  gives  in 
Easily  led 
Modest 

3  Passive  and  unaggressive 
Meek 

Obeys  too  willingly 

4  Spineless 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  ADJECTIVE  CHECK  LIST 


457 


Octant  6:  JK 
J:    1  Grateful 

2  Admires  and  imitates  others 
Often  helped  by  others 
Very  respectful  to  authority 

3  Dependent 
Wants  to  be  led 
Hardly  ever  talks  back 

4  Clinging  vine 

Octant  7:  LM 
L:  1  Cooperative 

2  Eager  to  get  along  with  others 
Always  pleasant  and  agreeable 
Wants  everyone  to  like  him 

3  Too  easily  influenced  by  friends 
Will  confide  in  anyone 
Wants  everyone's  love 

4  Agrees  with  everyone 

Octant  8:  NO 
N:  I  Considerate 

2  Encouraging  others 
Kind  and  reassuring 
Tender  and  soft-hearted 

3  Forgives  anything 
Oversympathetic 

Too  lenient  with  others 

4  Tries  to  comfort  everyone 


1  Appreciative 

2  Very  anxious  to  be  approved  of 
Accepts  advice  readily 
Trusting  and  eager  to  please 

3  Lets  others  make  decisions 
Easily  fooled 

Likes  to  be  taken  care  of 

4  Will  believe  anyone 


M:  1  Friendly 

2  Affectionate  and  understanding 
Sociable  and  neighborly 
Warm 

3  Fond  of  everyone 
Likes  everybody 
Friendly  all  the  time 

4  Loves  everyone 


1  Helpful 

2  Big-hearted  and  unselfish 
Enjoys  taking  care  of  others 
Gives  freely  of  self 

3  Generous  to  a  fault 
Overprotective  of  others 
Too  willing  to  give  to  others 

4  Spoils  people  with  kindness 


Derivation  of  the  Interpersonal  Adjective  Check  List 

An  attempt  was  made  to  develop  a  stimulus  situation  which  would  be 
a  balanced  representation,  at  various  intensities,  of  each  of  the  sixteen 
hypothesized  varieties  of  interpersonal  behavior.  In  assigning  scores  to  the 
test  responses,  the  "unit"  assumed  to  be  invariant  became,  not  the  standard 
deviation  computed  for  a  certain  sample  under  certain  scaling  assump- 
tions, but  an  event  from  a  defined  set  of  events;  the  subject's  selection  or 
rejection  of  any  word  in  the  list.  The  advantage  of  such  an  approach  is 
that  direct  numerical  comparison  of  raw  scores  (number  of  words  in  a 
given  category  checked  by  a  subject)  is  possible  and  meaningful  as  a  set 
of  communications  from  the  patient,  so  that  a  model  for  statistical  in- 
ference need  involve  no  untestable  scaling  assumptions.  As  a  result, 
idiographic  procedures,  similar  to  Stephenson's  Q-technique,  became 
applicable.  The  disadvantage  is  that  the  selection  of  each  item  becomes  of 
crucial  importance. 

Because  the  interpersonal  system  is  at  present  typically  more  a  method 
than  a  set  of  entities  or  measurements,  any  process  of  criterion  item  selec- 
tion seemed  likely  to  be  premature  and  limiting.  Instead,  a  priori  selection 
of  words  by  a  conference  of  from  four  to  six  psychologists  was  followed 


4j8  APPENDICES 

by  a  posteriori  analysis  of  the  way  in  which  these  words  were  actually 
used  by  the  patients.  Both  the  intuitive  judgments  and  the  empirical 
check  were  essential  aspects  of  the  developmental  process. 

Development  and  Revision  of  the  Interpersonal  Check  List 

General  Background.  The  development  of  the  Interpersonal  Check 
List  has  taken  place  over  a  period  of  four  y fears.  Four  major  forms  have 
been  developed  successively,  the  third  having  been  twice  revised. 

The  initial  source  of  items  was  a  334  adjective  check  list  prepared  by 
Suczek  to  be  representative  of  trait  lists  extant  in  psychological  literature 
up  to  1950.  Form  I  was  a  selection  of  106  interpersonal  words  made  from 
this  list  on  the  basis  of  the  pooled  judgments  of  five  psychologists.  .  .  . 
The  goals  of  the  first  revision  were  to  obtain  a  fuller  and  more  even  rep- 
resentation of  the  varieties  of  interpersonal  behavior.  Balance  among  the 
sixteen  categories  in  the  frequency  of  "yes"  responses  obtained  has  been 
improved  with  each  successive  form,  but  in  later  revisions  attention  was 
concentrated  on  the  meaning  of  the  text  items  for  each  patient.  .  .  . 

Samples  Tested.  During  the  three  year  period  of  revision,  the  check 
list  has  been  administered  to  several  thousand  subjects  in  a  variety  of  ways. 
The  principal  use  has  been  as  part  of  the  evaluation  procedure  for  incom- 
ing patients  to  the  psychiatric  clinic.  Other  samples  include  several  hun- 
dred students  at  the  University  of  California,  Berkeley;  100  students  at 
San  Francisco  State  College,  San  Francisco;  a  group  of  dermatitis  patients 
from  the  practice  of  Dr.  Herbert  Lawrence,  in  San  Francisco;  and  a  group 
of  200  overweight  women.  The  most  frequent  administration  has  called 
for  a  description  of  self.  The  majority  of  subjects  have  also  been  asked 
to  describe  their  mother,  father,  spouse,  and  (for  Forms  lib  and  IV)  their 
ideal  self.  In  addition,  some  subjects  have  been  asked  to  use  the  list  to 
describe  people  in  general,  to  describe  the  characters  in  their  TAT  stories, 
and  to  describe  the  other  members  of  their  therapy  group,  i.e.,  as  a  form 
of  sociometric.  The  statistical  data  used  for  revision  of  the  check  list 
have  been  derived  from  these  samples. 

Problems  and  Methods  of  Revision.  Once  the  decision  to  discard 
standard  scores  and  other  scaling  devices  had  been  made,  it  was  necessary 
to  manipulate  item  content  in  order  to  develop  comparability  among 
variables.  One  difficulty  was  the  variation  in  the  frequency  of  "yes"  re- 
sponses among  the  sixteen  categories.  After  a  study  of  both  the  frequen- 
cies with  which  individual  words  on  Form  I  were  checked  and  the  average 
raw  scores  obtained  in  the  various  sixteenth  and  octant  categories,  it  was 
decided  that  the  introduction  of  an  explicit  "intensity"  dimension  would 
simplify  the  attainment  of  approximately  comparable  raw  scores.  For 
Form  II,  words  were  rated  either  2  or  5  on  a  four-point  scale  from  /  "A 
mild  or  necessary  amount  of  a  trait"  to  4,  "An  extreme  or  highly  inap- 
propriate amount."  Three  words  in  each  interpersonal  category  were  of 
intensity  2,  "Moderate  or  appropriate,"  while  three  were  of  intensity  5, 
"Marked  or  inappropriate."  In  Forms  III  and  IV,  words  or  phrases  fitting 
all  four  degrees  of  intensity  were  used. 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  ADJECTIVE  CHECK  LIST  459 

A  second  and  related  task  was  to  minimize  the  effect  of  certain  ex- 
traneous determinants  of  the  test  scores.  Among  these  are  the  misunder- 
standing or  failure  to  recognize  the  meaning  of  a  word,  the  selection  of 
an  alternate  meaning  differing  from  our  usage,  the  general  tendency  to 
mark  more  or  fewer  words,  and  the  tendency  to  check  more  or  fewer 
"good"  (positive  valued)  words.  The  latter  tendency  could  be  considered 
an  error  if  it  reflected  a  misapprehension  of  the  testing  situation  rather 
than  a  habit  of  looking  at  oneself  and  others  with  such  a  bias.  We  con- 
sidered all  these  effects  to  be  special  cases  of  the  larger  problem  of  dif- 
ferences in  set,  which  unquestionably  affect  test  performance,  producing 
superficially  derived  effects  often  attributed  directly  to  underlying  per- 
sonality structures.  Our  thought  was  that  interviews  following  the  test 
sessions  would  be  the  most  fruitful  method  of  evaluating  the  effect  of 
differing  sets  on  performance  in  the  clinic  setting,  and  such  interviews 
were  begun  with  the  construction  of  Form  IIL 

However,  even  without  such  interviews,  it  was  clear  from  the  evidence 
at  hand  that  certain  general  shifts  with  respect  to  intensity  and  value  were 
necessary.  For  one  thing,  although  tallies  of  the  octant  scores  showed 
marked  differences  depending  on  application,  intensity  and  sample,  .  .  . 
it  was  clear  that  all  subjects  were  checking  more  words  on  the  right-hand 
(friendly)  side  of  the  circle  than  on  the  left  (hostile).  To  some  extent 
this  was  a  valid  representation  reflecting  the  inhibition  of  hostile  expres- 
sion in  our  culture,  but  largely  it  seemed  to  reflect  a  difference  in  set 
between  patients  who  were  using  the  words  to  describe  themselves  and 
psychologists  who  were  judging  the  words  from  a  vantage  of  psycho- 
pathological  theory. 

To  correct  this  bias,  words  were  rated  with  respect  to  their  value  in  the 
patient  culture.^  Then  new  intensity  ratings  were  given  the  words  with 
an  eye  to  their  rated  value  and  to  the  frequency  with  which  they  had  been 
checked  by  the  patients.  For  example,  appreciative  and  cooperative  had 
been  checked  by  nearly  everyone,  and  so  were  scaled  down  in  intensity 
from  2  to  1.  Opposite  adjustments  were  necessary  on  the  left  side  of  the 
circle.  The  empirical  relation  between  the  intensity  assigned  to  an  item 
and  the  frequency  of  patients'  "yes"  responses  to  the  item  was  used  in 
later  forms  to  correct  mis-scored  items.  Considerable  scatter  within  an 
intensity  occurred,  but  the  rough  rule  was  set  up  that  intensity  1  words 
should  be  answered  "yes"  by  about  90%  of  the  population,  intensity  2  by 
about  67%,  intensity  3  by  about  33%,  and  intensity  4  by  about  10%. 
Boundaries  were  set  between  these  points,  and  items  deviating  too  greatly 
were  eliminated  or  moved  to  a  more  appropriate  intensity. 

Many  words,  like  discriminating,  conciliatory,  although  agreed  upon 
by  raters  and  patients,  had  to  be  discarded  because  they  were  unintelligible 
to  a  sizable  proportion  of  patients.  Others,  like  demanding,  were  suscep- 
tible of  differences  in  interpretation  which  made  ambiguous  their  inter- 

*  Ratings  of  value  were  done  on  a  three-point  scale  according  as  the  typical  pa- 
tient would  consider  the  word  in  question  as  describing  a  "good,"  "neutral,"  or  "bad" 
trait. 


46o  APPENDICES 

personal  score.  Our  evidence  in  these  cases  was  the  tallies  of  words  which 
patients  had  complained  about  or  marked  as  ones  not  understood  and  the 
intercorrelations  of  a  word  with  the  other  words  in  the  list.  Some  words 
were  simply  annoying  to  patients  for  other  reasons  and  were  discarded. 
An  example  is  pollyanna.  To  develop  adequate  item  clarity  and  precision, 
brief  phrases  were  introduced  if  no  single  adjective  could  be  found,  al- 
though items  were  kept  as  brief  as  possible  in  the  interests  of  speed  and 
ease  of  test-taking. 

In  the  revision  of  Form  Illb,  the  pattern  of  intercorrelations  of  each 
item  with  all  other  iteTm  of  the  same  frequency  was  the  most  important 
source  of  information.^  A  good  item  was  characterized  by  high  correla- 
tions with  neighboring  items  and  low  correlations  with  items  more  distant 
on  the  circle  of  variables.  It  is  customary  in  validating  items  to  use  the 
score  on  a  set  of  items  as  a  criterion.  We  were  able  to  go  directly  to  the 
item-intercorrelations  themselves  because  we  were  comparing  items  of 
the  same  relative  frequency  and  because  the  whole  pattern  of  intercorrela- 
tions had  meaning.  Since  a  poor  item  was  associated  not  only  with  a  poor 
pattern  on  its  own  graph,  but  also  with  a  misplaced  point  on  the  graphs 
of  other  items,  the  effect  of  poor  items  could  be  discounted  in  our  con- 
siderations as  soon  as  one  had  been  identified.  Such  adjustment  is  not  pos- 
sible if  a  score  is  used  as  an  item  criterion.  In  some  cases,  the  majority  of 
items  in  a  category  were  found  to  be  poor.  For  these  cases,  the  criterion 
itself  would  have  selected  poor  items.  Approximately  6,000  item  inter- 
correlations were  examined  in  the  revision  of  Form  Illb,  and  replacements 
were  made  for  items  having  poor  patterns. 

Our  fairly  standard  procedure  at  each  revision  was  to  collect  statistical 
data  on  the  previous  form:  the  frequencies  with  which  the  individual 
words  in  each  octant,  16th  and  intensity  were  being  checked  by  the  var- 
ious samples  in  the  several  situations,  the  average  test  scores  for  each  sam- 
ple, the  tally  of  words  which  the  patients  had  marked  as  ones  not  under- 
stood, together  with  a  summary  of  their  verbal  complaints,  the  octant 
intercorrelations,  and,  for  Form  Illb,  the  item  intercorrelations.  To  these 
data  were  added  the  five  psychologists'  ratings  of  each  word  with  respect 
to  interpersonal  category  and  intensity.  Psychologists'  opinions  as  to  the 
understandability  and  over-all  desirability  of  each  word  were  also  re- 
corded. Each  word  was  then  considered  by  a  conference  of  from  four 
to  six  psychologists.  Changes  in  the  list  were  of  several  types:  the  dis- 
carding of  a  word  or  phrase,  assignment  of  a  new  intensity  of  16th  desig- 
nation, or  a  modification  in  wording.  The  remaining  list  of  satisfactory  or 
modified  words  was  used  as  a  core  for  the  new  form  of  the  check  list.  To 
this  were  added  new  words  or  phrases  as  required.  Thesauri,  as  well  as  the 

2  It  was  possible  to  examine  such  a  quantity  of  data  because  the  test  responses 
were  already  punched  into  IBM  cards.    Actually,  an  approximation  to  the  inter- 

correlations  was  used,  namely  *      „  ,      for  a  given  ;. 

For  roughly  constant  marginal  frequencies,  this  approximation  is  good. 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  ADJECTIVE  CHECK  LIST  461 

individual  and  collective  inspiration  of  the  staff,  were  used  as  sources  of 
words  which  would  be  meaningful  to  all  patients  in  the  exact  sense  desired. 

Experimental  forms  were  now  drawn  up  and  administered  to  patients 
for  two  or  three  weeks  of  normal  clinic  intake.  During  this  time,  inter- 
views about  the  test  items  were  conducted.  Patients  who  had  just  taken 
the  test  were  asked  to  point  out  words  which  seemed  unclear,  ambiguous, 
or  in  any  way  bothersome.  They  were  also  asked  to  define  specific  words 
about  which  we  had  some  uncertainties.  Finally,  a  general  evaluation  of 
and  reaction  to  the  test  was  requested.  On  the  basis  of  the  data  gathered 
with  the  experimental  form  a  revision  was  given  definitive  form. 

As  is  suggested  by  the  above,  the  process  of  development  of  the  ICL  is 
a  continuing  one,  but  it  was  felt  that  sufficient  progress  has  been  made  to 
justify  publication  of  the  present  Form  IV. 

Results 

Internal  Consistency.  Test-retest  reliability  correlations  are  avail- 
able on  77  of  the  obesity  sample  who  were  retested  after  an  interval  of 


TABLE  41 

Test-Retest  Correlations,  Form  IIIa,  by 

(Obesity  Sample,  N  = 

Octant  and  Sixteenth 

77) 

Octanti 

AP      BC 

DE 

FG      HI       JK 

LM      NO 

.76        .76 

.81 

.73         .78        .83 

Average  =  .78 

Sixteenths 

.15        .80 

A 

B      C      D      E 

F 

G      H      I       J 

K      L      M      N      0 

P 

.75 

.68     .74     .83     .76 

.64 

.77     .65     .76     .73 
Average  =  .73 

.76     .66     .73     .75     .74 

.69 

two  weeks.  Because  this  sample  is  a  somewhat  homogeneous  all-female 
group,  these  correlations  are  not  likely  to  be  larger  than  ones  obtainable 
with  other  groups.  On  the  other  hand,  obese  women  may  have  more 
stable  self  pictures  than  many  individuals.  The  sort  of  unreliability  which 
results  from  changes  in  one's  view  of  self  is  not  of  course  undesirable  in 
a  test  designed  to  depict  view  of  self.  Therefore  the  correlations  in 
Table  41,  which  average  .73  for  I6th  reliability  and  .78  for  octant  relia- 
bility, may  be  thought  of  as  suggesting  that  ICL  scores  can  have  suffi- 
cient stability  to  be  useful  in  personality  research  and  clinical  evaluation. 
Perhaps  more  important  than  reliabilities  are  the  intervariable  correla- 
tions. As  in  most  theories,  certain  relationships  among  variables  are  postu- 
lated, but  in  the  Interpersonal  System  these  relationships  are  particularly 
accessible  to  the  psychometrician.  For  example,  adjacent  variables  on  the 
circular  continuum  are  more  closely  related  than  non-adjacent,  and  the 
relationship  between  two  variables  is  a  monotonic  decreasing  function  of 


462  APPENDICES 

their  separation.  Empirical  measures  of  relationship,  such  as  the  correla- 
tion coefficient,  offer  an  opportunity  to  check  how  well  the  postulated 
order  holds.  Interoctant  and  intersixteenth  correlations  have  been  ob- 
tained on  several  samples,  and  these  correlations  are  summarized  in  Table 
42.  In  Table  42,  the  averages  of  the  correlations  for  variables  one  step  apart, 
two  steps  apart,  etc.,  are  shown.  It  is  evident  that  these  averages  decrease 
as  more  distant  variables  are  correlated.  Thus  observations  made  with  the 
check  list  confirm  that  a  roughly  circular  arrangement  of  the  variables 
can  be  used  to  describe  their  degree  of  relationship  to  one  another. 

TABLE  42 

Average  Intervariable  Correlation  as  a  Function  of  Their  Separation 
Around  the  Circle 

Data  Recorded  in  Sixteenths  (Raw  Scores) 


i 

^ntervariable  Distance 

Sample 

Form 

N         1         2 

3         4 

5         6 

7         8 

Obesity  females 

111 

77             .46       .37 

.34       .28 

.24      .21 

.19       .12 

Psychiatric  outpatient 
males 

Ilia 

76            .56       .48 

.36      .26 

.13       .11 

.06       .06 

Psychiatric  outpatient 
females 

Ilia 

122             .51       .39 

.25       .13 

.03  -.06 

-.14  -.19 

Data  Recorded  in  Octants 

Intervariable  Distance 

Sample 

Form 

Type  of  Score 

N 

1         2 

3         4 

Obesity  females 

III 

Raw  Scores 

77 

.51       .37 

.22       .12 

Psychiauic 
outpatient  males 
and  females 

II 

Raw  Scores 

83 

.60      .35 

.24       .11 

Psychiatric 
outpatient  males 
and  females 

II 

Divided  by 
number  of 
words  checked 

85 

.28  -.08 

-.44  —.48 

One  might  ask  why  no  appreciable  negative  correlations  are  reported 
among  raw  scores,  even  though  variables  opposite  in  meaning  are  corre- 
lated. The  answer  is  to  be  found  in  the  fact  that  all  variables  are  con- 
taminated by  a  common  factor,  the  over-all  likelihood  of  a  "yes"  response, 
regardless  of  item  content.  When  this  factor  is  removed  by  dividing  each 
raw  score  by  the  total  number  of  "yes"  responses  made  by  a  subject,  nega- 
tive correlations  do  in  fact  appear  whenever  variables  with  opposite  com- 
ponents of  meaning  are  correlated.  (Compare  the  last  two  lines  of  Table 
42.)  There  does  not  seem  to  be  any  particular  advantage  to  the  division 
of  scores  by  number  of  words  used  if  the  effect  of  this  over-all  "yes" 
tendency  is  kept  in  mind.  For  example,  the  correlations  of  raw  or  divided 
scores  with  MMPI  variables  have  the  same  average  absolute  value,  although 
in  individual  situations  one  or  the  other  type  of  score  may  yield  a  higher 
correlation. 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  ADJECTIVE  CHECK  LIST 


463 


Tentative  Clinic  Norms.  No  adequate  normative  data  on  a  variety  of 
samples  are  yet  available.  The  means  and  standard  deviations  presented  in 
Table  43  can  be  used  as  approximate  norms  for  clinical  samples.  They  rep- 
resent the  performance  of  all  patients  tested  during  six  months'  routine 
intake  at  the  Permanente  Psychiatric  Clinic.  (1,  pp.  98-107) 

TABLE  43 
ICL  Means  and  Standard  Deviations  for  Psychiatric  Outpatients 


Form  Illb 

(144  items) 

Form  IV  (128  items) 

Octant 

Means          Standard  Deviations 

Means          Standard  Deviations 

Males 

Females 

Males 

Females 

Males 

Females 

Males 

Females 

N  =  75  N  =  137 

N  =  75  N  =  137 

N  =  86  N  =  152 

N  =  86  N  =  152 

AP 

8.5 

7.7 

2.6 

3.4 

6.2 

6.2 

3.0 

3.1 

BC 

8.3 

6.9 

2.5 

2.9 

6.3 

5.7 

2.8 

2.5 

DE 

8.6 

7.6 

2.9 

3.4 

7.4 

7.1 

3.2 

2.9 

FG 

8.7 

9.4 

3.6 

3.7 

7.3 

7.7 

3.2 

3.4 

HI 

9.6 

9.6 

3.1 

3.5 

7.0 

7.8 

3.3 

3.3 

JK 

9.2 

8.8 

2.7 

2.3 

7.2 

8.1 

2.6 

3.0 

LM 

U.O 

10.6 

3.2 

3.1 

7.1 

8.2 

3.0 

3.1 

NO 

9.3 

9.4 

4.3 

3.4 

6.4 

7.7 

3.0 

3.8 

Vector  Sum 

DOM 

-1.3 

-3.2 

7.0 

8.5 

-2.0 

-3.4 

7.9 

7.8 

LOV 

3.5 

4.4 

9.0 

9.1 

-0.4 

2.8 

8.5 

8.9 

References 

1.  LaForge,  R.,  and  R.  Suczek.  The  interpersonal  dimension  of  personality:  III.  An 
interpersonal  check  list.  /.  Pers.,  1955,  24,  No.  1,  94-112. 

2.  LaForge,  R.  Notes  on  the  application  of  I.  B.  M.  techniques  to  the  analysis  of 
psychological  data.  Unpublished  paper  at  Oakland,  CaHf.:  Kaiser  Foundation 
Psychology  Research,  1954. 


The  Administration,  Scoring,  and 
Validation  of  the  Level  III-TAT 


The  instrument  employed  routinely  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  clinical 
and  research  studies  for  measuring  Level  III  behavior  is  the  Thematic 
Apperception  Test  (2).  This  appendix  presents  some  pertinent  tech- 
nical and  research  information  relating  to  the  Level  III-TAT.  The 
details  of  administration  will  be  presented.  The  scoring  of  the  TAT 
protocols  in  terms  of  the  sixteen-variable  system  vi^ill  then  be  dis- 
cussed. A  guide  for  assigning  interpersonal  ratings  to  the  stories  most 
commonly  eUcited  is  included.  The  basic  research  study  which  dem- 
onstrates the  ability  of  Level  III-TAT  to  predict  future  changes  in 
overt  behavior  will  then  be  reviewed. 

Ten  TAT  stimulus  cards  are  routinely  employed.  These  cards 
were  selected  on  the  basis  of  their  interpersonal  connotations.  Cards 
used  for  males  and  females  are: 

Males  Females 

1  1 

2  2 
3BA/I  3GF 
4                                                           4 
6BM                                                   6BM 
6GF                                                   6GF 
7BM                                                   7GF 
12M  12M 
13MF  13MF 
18BM  18GF 

Administration 

The  TAT  is  administered  in  a  group  testing  situation.  The  patients 
are  given  ten  TAT  cards  (face  down)  and  several  sheets  of  blank 

464 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT  465 

paper.  They  are  instructed  to  make  up  a  story  about  the  people  in  the 
TAT  stimulus  picture.  They  are  told  that  there  should  be  interaction 
among  the  people  in  their  stories.  They  are  to  state  what  the  situation 
is,  how  the  figures  feel  about  each  other,  and  how  the  situation  is  re- 
solved. 

While  the  subjects  are  writing  their  stories,  the  tester  checks  their 
responses  to  the  first  cards  to  make  sure  that  the  instructions  are  being 
followed.  The  average  psychiatric  clinic  patient  (mean  years  of 
schooling  for  women  =  12.4,  men  =  13.2)  completes  the  ten  card 
TAT  in  forty-five  minutes. 

Scoring  of  Interpersonal  Themes  from  TAT  Stories 

The  TAT  protocols  are  then  rated  by  three  independent  judges 
who  assign  one  or  more  interpersonal  themes  to  the  hero  of  each  story 
and  to  the  "other"  personages. 

The  first  step  in  the  scoring  process  is  the  determination  of  the 
hero  figure.  The  TAT  personage  who  is  the  central  figure  in  the 
story  is  designated  the  hero.  There  are  criteria  used  to  make  this 
decision. 

( 1 )  The  figure  with  whom  the  subject  is  most  involved  is  the  hero. 

(2)  The  figure  receiving  the  most  descriptive  space  is  the  hero. 

(3)  The  figure  who  parallels  the  subject's  age  and  sex  is  the  hero. 

These  criteria  are  listed  in  descending  order  of  importance.  In  most 
cases  it  is  not  necessary  to  apply  these  criteria — the  hero  is  obvious. 
Only  where  the  issue  of  centrality  is  in  doubt  are  these  three  criteria 
applied.  In  some  cases  both  characters  or  all  characters  mentioned  can 
be  considered  the  hero  if  no  distinction  or  separation  between  figures 
is  made  by  the  patient. 

The  second  step  in  scoring  a  TAT  story  is  to  assign  the  appropriate 
interpersonal  ratings  to  the  hero.  If  the  "other"  figures  are  attributed 
interpersonal  feelings  or  actions  these  are  then  scored.  Two  raters 
and  a  judge  are  used  to  score  the  TAT  protocols  which  are  being  used 
for  research  purposes.  For  routine  clinical  diagnosis  it  is  often  neces- 
sary to  rely  on  single  ratings. 

There  are  two  methods  for  rating  TAT  themes.  The  first  employs 
the  sixteen-variable  lettered  code  (B  =  narcissism,  C  =  exploitation, 
etc.).  The  second  employs  the  numerical  octant  codes  (2  =  narcis- 
sism-B  or  exploitation-C,  etc.).  Since  the  octant  scores  are  employed 
in  the  trigonometric  summary  formulas,  the  numerical  system  is  now 
routinely  used  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  system. 

A  strict  legislative  procedure  has  been  developed  for  the  judging 
process.  The  first  two  raters  make  their  scoring  decisions  independ- 


466  APPENDICES 

ently.  The  judge  then  inspects  these  ratings  and  makes  a  third  and  de- 
cisive rating  only  in  the  case  where  the  first  two  independent  raters 
are  in  disagreement.  The  judge  cannot  change  a  rating  if  the  first  two 
raters  agree  on  the  same  octant  score.  If  the  first  two  raters  disagree 
on  the  octant  score  the  judge  then  has  the  authority  to  agree  with 
either  of  the  raters  or  to  substitute  a  third  rating.  The  judge's  ruling 
is  final. 

In  some  cases  the  first  two  raters  assign  more  than  one  score  to  any 
TAT  figure.  They  may  agree  on  one  score  but  disagree  on  the  second 
score.  A  rule  has  been  developed  to  handle  this  eventuality.  If  there 
is  any  disagreement  in  scores  assigned  to  a  TAT  figure,  the  judge  has 
the  right  to  change  all  the  scores  assigned  to  that  figure. 

A  manual  has  been  prepared  to  assist  in  the  training  of  TAT  raters. 
Table  44  presents  the  most  typical  themes  assigned  to  the  figure  in  the 
ten-card  TAT  employed  by  the  Kaiser  Foundation  project.  Percent- 
age figures  indicating  the  relative  frequency  of  appearance  of  these 
themes  are  included  in  Table  44.^  These  percentages  are  sometimes 
reflections  of  the  figure  chosen  as  Hero.  They  are  included  for  didactic 
purposes  only. 

TABLE  44 
Guide  to  Assigning  Interpersonal  Ratings  to  Ten 
TAT  Stories  (Level  III-T) 
CARD  1 

Hero  (Little  Boy) 


(a)  The  boy  feels  rebellious  and  passively  resists  his  parents 
wishes  that  he  play  the  violin. 

(b)  He  experiences  success  in  later  life. 

(c)  He  is  daydreaming. 

[The  score  /  is  also  assigned  when  the  boy  is  conform- 
ing to  his  parents  wishes.  If  he  is  dreaming  about  suc- 
cess, he  gets  the  double  score  for  passivity  and 
achievement.] 

(d)  He  feels  left  out  and  unhappy. 

(e)  He  has  feelings  of  failure  or  impotence. 

(f)  He  asserts  his  will  actively,  often  against  his  parents. 

Other  (Parents  or  Parent  Figures) 


(a)  The  parents  are  forcing  the  boy  to  play  the  violin. 

(b)  They  give  the  boy  the  violin  or  suppon  him  in  his  efforts. 

(c)  They  are  punitive  or  unsympathetic. 

1  The  percentage  scores  contained  in  Table  44  were  derived  by  John  Enright  of 
the  University  of  California  and  Joan  S.  Harvey  of  the  Kaiser  Foundation  staff. 


Hero 

Letter 

Numerical 

% 

Code 

Code 

F 

4 

39 

P 

1 

27 

J 

6 

11 

}  and  P 

61 

H 

1 

n 

21 

B 

2 
Other 

3 

Rating 

% 

A 

47 

»rts. 

NotO 

19 

DotE 

14 

Rating 

% 

B 

28 

ForG 

21 

J 

11 

L 

7 

O 

15 

B2ndH 

Other 

Rating 

% 

BotC 

29 

NotO 

19 

D 

13 

J 

4 

THE  LEVEL  III-TAT  467 

CARD    2 

Hero   (Girl  with  Books) 


(a)  Tlie  girl  feels  or  acts  independently. 

(b)  She  feels  rebellious,  resentful,  or  deprived. 

(c)  She  conforms  and  gives  up  her  independent  strivings. 

(d)  She  adjusts  herself  agreeably,  and  a  happy  ending  results. 

(e)  She  gives  things  to  or  supports  her  parents. 

(f)  She  asserts  independence  but  regrets  decision  in  later  life. 

Other  (Usually  Family  Members) 


(a)  The  family  members  are  selfish  or  rejecting  [most  common  score 
for  neurotic  groups]. 

(b)  They  are  generous  and  sympathetic. 

(c)  They  are  angry  because  the  girl  is  rebellious. 

(d)  They  are  docile,  conforming  people. 

Comments:  If  the  family  members  are  mentioned  in  a  descriptive  sense  they  are 
not  scored  (e.g.,  man  plowing,  woman  is  pregnant).  If  their  activities  put  inter- 
personal pressure  on  the  girl  they  are  given  the  appropriate  score  (e.g.,  the  man's 
conformity  contrasted  to  the  heroine's  desire  for  independence,  etc.). 

BM 
CARD  3  GF 

Hero  (Solitary  Figure) 
[Since  there  are  no  differences  in  the  percentages  of  scores  assigned  to  Cards  3  GF 
and  3  BM,  they  are  considered  together.] 


(a)  The  Hero  is  depressed,  defeated,  suicidal,  etc. 

(b)  The  Hero  is  bitter,  disappointed,  exploited. 

(c)  The  Hero  resolves  the  problem  through  conformity  or  trust. 

(d)  A  happy  ending. 

[The  last  two  scores  involve  generally  subsidiary  or  outcome  themes.] 
Others 


(a)  The  world  is  rejecting. 

(b)  The  world  is  punitive  or  sadistic. 

(c)  The  world  is  sympathetic  and  helpful. 

CARD  4 

Hero  (Man  or  Woman*) 

(a)  The  man  is  striving  for  independence  or  rejecting  the  woman. 

(b)  Feelings  of  despair,  indecisiveness,  guilt,  and  immobility. 

(c)  The  woman  dependently  pleads  with  the  man. 

(d)  The  man  has  angry  or  punitive  feelings. 

(e)  The  man  is  involved  in  rebellious  or  criminal  activities. 

(f)  The  woman  forcibly  (or  in  a  wise  manner)  pleads  with  the  man 

not  to  engage  in  impulsive  or  rash  activity.  ^  or  P 

•  Comment:  The  "Hero-Other"  distinction  varies  according  to  sex.  We  have  there- 
fore designated  the  most  common  themes  without  "Hero"  and  "Other."  For  this  rea- 
son no  per  cent  figures  can  be  cited. 


Hero 

Rating 

% 

H 

46 

ForG 

23 

/orK 

4 

L 
1 

3 

Other 

Rating 

% 

c 

31 

DotE 

38 

NoiO 

23 

Rating 

BoiC 

H(l) 

JotK 

DotE 

ForG 

H       27 


C        18 


.gg  APPENDICES 

CARD  6  BM 

Hero  (Man) 

Hero 
Rating    % 

(a)  The  man  (son)  strives  for  independence   (e.g.,  towards  marriage, 

career,  etc.).  ^        35 

(b)  The  man  feels   guilt  and   unhappiness    (often   accompanying   the 
separation  from  the  mother). 

(c)  The  man  is  bitter,  rebellious,  engaged  in  crime,  etc.  ForG    11 

Other  (VN'^oman) 

Other 
Rating    % 
(a)    The  mother  is  unhappy  because  of  the  son's  departure  or  wrong- 
doing (or  because  of  bad  news).  H       26 
<b)    The  mother  attempts  directly  or  indirectly  to  prevent  the  son  from 
leaving  her. 

(c)  The  mother  is  hurt  or  bitter.  ForG    17 

(d)  The  mother  gives  blessing.  N 

(e)  The  mother  eventually  accepts  situation.  / 

(f )  She  learns  to  love  daughter-in-law,  and  all  are  happy.  L 
[(e)  and  (f )  scores  assigned  to  outcome  themes.] 

CARD  6  OF 

[The  Hero  on  this  card  generally  varies  according  to  the  sex  of  the  subject. 
The  most  typical  responses  for  females  will  therefore  be  listed  separately  from  the 
males.] 

Females 

Hero 
Rating    % 

(a)  The  woman  is  fearfully  surprised  by  the  man.  H        17 

(b)  She  is  rejecting  or  refusing  the  man.  BorC    23 

(c)  She  is  pleasantly  surprised  by  the  man's  offer.  JorK    10 

(d)  She  has  committed  a  rebellious  or  deceitful  act.  Feels  bitter  or  dis- 
satisfied. F  or  G    14 

Males 

(a)  The  man  is  surprising  the  woman  for  exploitive  or  seductive  pur- 
poses. C       28 

(b)  He  is  surprising  the  woman  with  an  offer  of  tenderness  or  generos-  N  orO  28 
ity  or  love.  M 

(c)  He  is  accusing  her  of  crimes  of  omission  or  commission.  D        15 

CARD  7  GF 

Hero  (Daughter) 

Hero 
Rating    % 

(a)  The  girl  is  docilely  listening  to  the  older  woman  (or  depending  on 

her).  JotK    28 

(b)  She  feels  rebellious,  bitter,  hurt,  or  passively  resistant.  ForG    16 

(c)  She  is  unhappy  or  fearful.  H  or  I    22 

(d)  She  grows  up  and  attains  success  and  motherhood.  P        12 


THE  LEVEL  in-TAT 


469 


Other  (Older  Woman) 


(a)  She  is  reading  to  or  advising  the  girl. 

(b)  She  is  comforting  or  helping  the  girl. 


Other 

Rating  % 

P  37 

O  33 


CARD  7  BM 


Hero  (Young  Man) 


(a)  Young  man  is  listening  to  or  asking  advice  from  the  older  man. 

(b)  He  is  bitter  or  rebelling  against  unsought-for  advice. 

(c)  He  feels  helpless  or  guUty. 

(d)  He  is  actively  resisting,  establishing  independence,  or  is  involved  in 
an  exploitative  maneuver. 

Other  (Older  Man) 


(a)  He  is  advising  the  young  man. 

(b)  Themes  of  arrogance  or  exploitation  are  attributed  to  the  older 
man. 

(c)  He  is  helping  or  supporting  the  young  man. 


Hero 
Rating 
JotK 

F 
Horl 


BotC    19 

Other 
Rating    % 
AotP   48 

BorC    19 
O       10 


CARD  12  M 


Hero  (Boy) 


(a)  The  boy  is  sick,  unconscious,  hypnotized,  or  asleep. 

(b)  He  docilely  or  dependently  pulls  help  from  the  other. 

Other  (Man) 


(a)  The  man  is  hypnotizing  or  exerting  power. 

(b)  He  is  helping,  curing,  praying  over,  or  tenderly  ministering. 

(c)  He  is  selfishly  exploiting  the  other. 


Hero 

Rating 

% 

I 

36 

JotK 

12 

Other 

Rating 

% 

AorP 

20 

NorO 

29 

BorC 

15 

CARD  13  MF 


Hero  (Man) 


(a)  The  man  is  unhappy,  despairing,  guilty,  immobilized. 

(b)  An  unconventional  or  immoral  act  has  occurred. 

(c)  A  murderous  or  sadistic  act. 

(d)  An  exploitive  or  selfish  action  (e.g.,  rape,  seduction,  taking  by  force 
from  the  other). 

Other  (Woman) 


(a)  The  woman  is  sick  or  exhausted. 

(b)  She  is  unconcerned  or  satisfied   (usually  in  contrast  to  the  man's 
sexual  guilt). 


Hero 

Rating 

% 

HOTI 

37 

F 

15 

DotE 

10 

BorC  16 

Other 

Rating  % 

I  36 

BorC  17 


470 

CARD  18  BM 


Hero  (Man) 


(a)  The  man  is  unconscious,  drunk,  passive,  defenseless,  etc. 

(b)  He  has  committed  a  criminal  or  rebellious  act. 

(c)  He  is  struggling  with  outside  forces. 

(d)  He  is  dependent  upon  others. 

Other  (Hands) 


(a)  Others  are  punishing,  attacking,  or  arresting  the  Hero. 

(b)  Othfxs  are  exploiting  or  manipulating  the  Hero  for  their  own  pur- 
poses. 

(c)  Others  are  helping,  protecting,  or  rescuing  the  Hero. 

(d)  Others  are  restraining  or  exerting  power  over  the  Hero  (where  the 

power  is  neither  clearly  hostile  nor  helpful).  AotP    11 

CARD  18  GF 

[Both  figures  in  this  card  can  play  the  Hero  role.   The  typical  themes  attributed 
to  each  figure  are  as  follows:] 


APPENDICES 

Hero 

Rating 

% 

HotI 

57 

F 

6 

B 

20 

JorK 

9 

Other 

Rating 

% 

DorE 

32 

pur- 

BorC 

29 

NorO 

25 

Top  Figure 


Rating 


(a)  The  woman  is  unhappy  because  of  the  illness  (or  injury)  of  the 
other.  H 

(b)  The  woman  is  angry  or  punitive  toward  the  other.  D  or  £ 

(c)  She  is  helping  the  injured  or  ill  other.  O 

(d)  She  is  suspicious  or  bitter  about  the  other's  behavior.  F  or  G 

Lower  Figure 

Rating 

(a)  The  lower  figure  is  injured  or  iU.  / 

(b)  She  is  hostile  to  or  fighting  with  the  other.  D  or  £ 

(c)  She  has  done  something  rebellious,  "wrong,"  or  sneaky.  F 

Management  of  TAT  Scores.  The  TAT  ratings  are  entered  by 
the  three  raters  on  a  scoring  form.  This  form  (see  Table  45)  contains 
boxes  for  the  two  raters  and  the  judge  to  record  their  decisions  and 
additional  space  for  performing  the  calculations  necessary  to  convert 
the  scores  into  summary  indices. 

After  the  rating  and  judging,  the  judge's  scores  (columns  on  the 
right  of  Table  45)  are  then  tallied  by  octant  (bottom  right  of  Table 
45).  The  octant  scores  are  then  fed  into  the  trigonometric  formulas 
(see  Chapter  6),  and  the  horizontal  and  vertical  indices  for  both 
"hero"  and  "other"  are  calculated. 

Subtotals  for  the  themes  attributed  to  males,  females,  and  maternal 
and  paternal  figures  can  also  be  entered  in  the  appropriate  boxes  of 
the  scoring  sheet.  The  lO-card  TAT  does  not  yield  enough  scores  to 
diagnose  these  fantasy  figures  reliably. 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT 


Subject. 


47 


TABLE  45 
Molar  Rating  Sheet 
Group  No TAT  No. 


« 

Hero 

Hero 
Role 

Other 

Other 
Role 

Hero 

Hero 
Role 

Other 

?^/  — 

Hero 
Role 

Other 

Other 
Role 

1 

2 

4 

6BM 

6GF 

7BM 

'gf 

12  M 

13  MF 

,„BM 

"gf 

Maternal  Id. 

Paternal  Id. 

Parental  Id. 

Cros 

-Sex 

Hero 

Other 

Total 

Hero 

Mother 

Hero 

Father 

Hero 

Parents 

Hero 

Other 

AP 
EC 
DE 
FG 
HI 
JK 
LM 
NO 

Total 

Dom 

Dom 

Lov 

Lov 

Other 

X 

a 

DOM 

-0.04 

2.41 

LOV 

-0.26 

2.59 

472  APPENDICES 

The  horizontal  and  vertical  indices  for  "hero"  and  for  "other"  are 
then  converted  into  standard  scores  and  plotted  on  the  diagnostic 
grid  to  determine  the  Level  III-T  diagnostic  categories. 

Norms  for  the  TAT.  The  vertical  and  horizontal  indices  for  Level 
III-T  are  converted  into  standard  scores  derived  from  a  sample  of  100 
consecutive  patients  tested  at  the  Permanente  Psychiatric  clinic.  The 
means  and  sigmas  of  the  normative  group  are  presented  in  Table  46. 

TABLE  46 

Means  and  Sigmas  of  Normative  Group  for  Level  III-T  Hero  and  "Other" 
(N  =  100) 

Hero 

X  a- 

DOM  —5.67  3.37 

LOV  +0.09  2.98 

The  Basic  Validation  Study  Demonstrating  That  Level  lll-TAT 
(Hero)  Predicts  Changes  in  Future  Overt  Behavior 

In  Chapter  8  (and  in  the  subsequent  chnical  chapters)  it  was  sug- 
gested that  fantasy  expressions  predict  personality  changes  which 
can  be  expected  in  the  future.  A  research  study  designed  to  test  this 
hypothesis  has  been  executed  and  will  now  be  reported.  The  subjects 
in  these  investigations  were  given  tests  of  conscious  self-description 
and  the  TAT  before  psychotherapy  (or  a  control  period).  Six  months 
later  the  test  of  conscious  self-description  was  repeated.  The  issue  is 
this:  did  the  TAT's  given  before  the  experimental  period  forecast  the 
amount  and  kind  of  change  which  took  place  over  time? 

Subjects.  Two  samples  of  subjects  were  employed  in  this  research. 
The  first  comprised  42  psychiatric  clinic  patients  who  were  tested  be- 
fore and  after  six  months  of  psychotherapy. 

There  were  no  apparent  extraneous  selective  factors  which  might 
artificially  influence  the  results. 

A  second  control  sample  was  also  studied:  81  are  female  subjects 
who  were  participating  in  a  weight-reduction  program  ^  and  were 
tested  before  and  after  a  six-month  period  in  which  they  attended  dis- 
cussion groups  on  dietary  problems.  These  groups  did  not  have  a 
psychotherapeutic  orientation.  Although  less  change  in  personality  oc- 
curred in  these  groups,  it  was  believed  that  an  important  test  of  the 
predictive  hypothesis  would  be  furnished  if  it  could  be  demonstrated 
that  the  TAT's  (before  the  experimental  period)  would  predict  what- 
ever changes  in  personality  did  take  place  in  these  subjects. 

^  Volunteers  for  a  group  weight-reduction  study  at  Hernck  Alemorial  Hospital, 
Berkeley,  California. 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT  473 

Method  of  Procedure.  All  subjects  in  the  study  were  initially  ad- 
ministered two  tests — the  TAT  and  the  Interpersonal  Check  List  on 
which  they  rated  their  conscious  perceptions  of  self.  The  construction 
and  validation  of  this  check  list  has  been  described  in  Appendix  2. 
The  check  list  scores  were  converted  into  the  sixteen  (eight  moderate, 
eight  intense)  summary  interpersonal  diagnostic  categories. 

Every  subject  in  this  study  thus  received  a  two-level  interpersonal 
diagnosis — one  for  his  conscious  self-description  derived  from  the 
check  list  and  one  for  his  private,  "preconscious"  behavior  derived 
from  the  TAT.  A  subject,  for  example,  on  the  basis  of  his  initial  test- 
ing might  receive  the  following  diagnoses:  Level  II  =  5;  Level  III  =  L 
This  indicates  that  his  self-descriptions  were  masochistic  and  self- 
eflFacing  while  his  TAT  heroes  were  autocratic  and  power-oriented. 

The  method  presented  in  Chapter  1 3  for  expressing  in  numerical 
indices  interlevel  differences  was  employed.  These  indices  denote  the 
kind  and  amount  of  discrepancy  between  the  two  levels  of  personality 
being  compared.  The  discrepancy  index  for  the  illustrative  subject  just 
considered  is:  Dominance-Submission  =  +112;  Love-Hate  =  +22; 
d  =  114  (see  Table  58,  Appendix  5).  These  figures  indicate  that 
the  subject's  TAT  hero  is  considerably  more  dominating  (  +  112) 
and  slightly  less  hostile  (+22)  than  his  conscious  self-description. 
The  over-all  discrepancy  between  the  two  levels  is  the  highest  pos- 
sible (114).  This  means  that  his  TAT  expresses  themes  which  are 
most  different  from  his  conscious  self-perceptions. 

Each  subject  in  the  study  was  administered  the  Interpersonal  Check 
List  after  approximately  six  months.  A  diagnostic  code  was  then 
derived  for  each  post-test.  The  illustrative  case  was  self-diagnosed 
(after  six  months  of  therapy)  as  a  narcissistic  personality,  code  =  2. 
The  preresearch  score  for  each  subject  at  Level  II  was  then  compared 
with  the  post-Level  II  score  by  means  of  the  same  "discrepancy" 
methodology.  This  yields  a  numerical  index  of  change  at  the  level  of 
conscious  self-description.  For  the  illustrative  case  the  Level  II  diag- 
nosis changed  from  5  to  2.  The  indices  for  this  change  are:  Domi- 
nance-Submission =  +103;  Love-Hate  ==  —21;  d  =  105.  These  fig- 
ures mean  that  the  subject  became  (in  his  self-regard)  much  stronger 
and  more  dominant  (  +  103)  and  slightly  more  hostile  (—21).  His 
over-all  amount  of  change  was  considerable  (105  in  a  possible  range 
of  Oto  114). 

For  each  patient  in  the  psychotherapy  sample  and  for  each  subject 
in  the  control  sample  the  two  sets  of  discrepancy  indices  were  ob- 
tained: the  preresearch  discrepancy  between  the  Interpersonal  Check 
List  and  the  TAT  and  the  discrepancy  between  the  pre-  and  post- 
check  lists. 


474 


APPENDICES 


Two  hypotheses  were  then  stated:  the  discrepancies  between  the 
conscious  self-description  and  the  TAT  from  the  initial  testing  would 
predict  (1)  the  kind  of  pre-post  change  in  self-description  and  (2) 
the  amount  of  pre-post  change  at  this  level.  In  the  illustrative  case 
the  TAT  did  predict  quite  closely  the  increased  dominance  in  the 
post-  check  list  (H-112  versus  H-103);  the  TAT  did  not  predict  the 
shift  on  the  Love-Hate  axis  (-f-22  versus  —21).  The  discrepancy  be- 
tween the  initial  check  list  and  TAT  did  predict  quite  accurately  the 
amount  of  over-all  change  in  Level  II  over  time  (114  versus  105). 

Results.  There  are  two  sets  of  results:  those  which  reflect  the  abil- 
ity of  the  TAT  to  predict  the  kind  of  change  to  be  expected  over  time 
and  those  which  test  the  TAT's  accuracy  in  predicting  the  amount  of 
change.  In  the  former  study  two  measures  are  involved,  the  Domi- 
nance-Submission discrepancy  and  the  Love-Hostility.  If  the  TAT 
expressed  more  dominance  than  the  initial  self-description  a  plus  (-f) 
score  is  obtained.  If  the  pre-post  discrepancy  in  the  self-description 
yielded  a  plus  score  on  dominance,  then  the  TAT  was  considered  to 
have  predicted  accurately  the  kind  of  change.  The  same  type  of  plus 
and  minus  (— )  measures  for  the  Love-Hostility  axis  were  similarly 
compared. 

The  results  for  the  psychotherapy  and  control  samples  are  pre- 
sented in  Tables  47,  48,  49,  50,  51,  and  52.  For  the  psychotherapy 
sample  the  TAT  does  not  predict  change  in  Level  II  Dominance-Sub- 
mission (Table  47),  but  it  does  forecast  change  in  hostility  attributed 

TABLE  47 

Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Dominance-Submission 
Between  Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change 
in   Self-Diagnosis  of  Dominance-Submission   on   Pre-Post  Tests  for   23   Psycho- 
therapy Patients 


TAT  More  Dominant  Than 
Initial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

TAT  More  Submissive 

Than  Initial  Conscious 

Self-Description 

+ 

- 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Dominant  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

10 

5 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Submissive  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

6 

2 

X^  IS  not  significant. 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT 


475 


TABLE  48 


Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Disc3iepancy  on  Dominance-Submission 

Between  Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change  in 

Self-Diagnosis  of  Dominance-Submission  on   Pre-Post  Tests  for  40  Discussion 

Group  Controls 


TAT  More  Dominant  Than 
Initial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

TAT  More  Submissive 

Than  Initial  Conscious 

Self-Description 

+ 

- 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Dominant  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

+ 

16 

7 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Submissive  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

5 

12 

X     =6  32;  p  =  .02. 

to  self  (Table  50).  For  the  control  sample  of  obese  discussion  group 
members,  the  TAT  does  predict  both  the  change  in  Dominance-Sub- 
mission (Table  48,  p  =  .02)  and  in  Love-Hostility  (Table  51,  p  = 
.05).  When  the  therapy  and  control  groups  are  combined,  the  TAT 
predicts  change  in  Dominance  (Table  49,  p  =  .10)  and  in  Love 
(Table  52,  p  =  .01). 

TABLE  49 

Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Dominance-Submission 

Between  Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change  in 

Self-Diagnosis  of  Dominance-Submission  on  Pre-Post  Tests  for  Combined  Samples 

OF  23  Psychotherapy  Patients  and  40  Obesity  Patients 


TAT  More  Dominant  Than 
Imtial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

TAT  More  Submissive 

Than  Initial  Conscious 

Self-Description 

- 

- 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Dominant  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

26 

12 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Submissive  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

11 

14 

X^   =3.71: 


47<5 


APPENDICES 


TABLE  50 


Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Love-Hostility  Between 
Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change  in  Self- 
Diagnosis  OF  Love-Hostility  on  Pre-Post  Tests  for  23  Psychotherapy  Patients 


TAT  Less  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

TAT  More  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

+ 

- 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
Less  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

5 

1 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

5 

12 

X     =  5.06;  p  =  .05. 

Several  comments  can  be  made  about  these  results.  The  N's  cited 
are  smaller  than  those  given  for  the  original  samples  because  in  the 
"direction-of-change"  study  those  patients  who  had  no  initial  Level 
II  versus  III  discrepancy  or  no  Level  II  pre-post  discrepancies  were 
omitted,  since  plus  or  minus  signs  were  not  available.  They  are  in- 
cluded below  in  the  "amount-of-change"  study  where  "no-change" 
becomes  a  most  significant  statistic. 

TABLE  51 

Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Love-Hostility  Between 
Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change  in  Self- 
Diagnosis  of  Love-Hostility  on  Pre-Post  Tests  for  40  Discussion  Group  Controls 


TAT  Less  Hostile  Than 

Initial  Conscious  Self- 

Description 

TAT  More  HosUle  Than 

Initial  Conscious  Self- 

Descnption 

+ 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
Less  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

11 

10 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

4 

15 

X*  =4.18;  p  =  .05. 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT 


477 


TABLE  52 


Chi-Square  Relating  the  Kind  of  Initial  Discrepancy  on  Love-Hostility  Between 
Conscious  Self-Diagnosis  and  TAT  Diagnosis  to  the  Kind  of  Change  in  Self- 
Diagnosis   OF  Love-Hostility   on   Pre-Post   Tests   for   Combined   Samples   of    23 
Psychotherapy  Patients  and  40  Obesity  Patients 


TAT  Less  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Conscious  Self- 
Description 

TAT  More  Hostile  Than 

Initial  Conscious  Self- 

Description 

- 

- 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
Less  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

16 

11 

Post  Self-Diagnosis 
More  Hostile  Than 
Initial  Self-Diagnosis 

- 

9 

27 

X    =  7  57;  p  =  .01 


The  fact  that  the  TAT  works  better  as  a  predictive  instrument  for 
the  control  group  than  the  psychotherapy  group  is  somewhat  puzzling. 
It  may  be  explained  by  the  fact  that  the  psychotherapy  patients  were 
retested  during  a  period  of  greater  personality  change  (1).  Our  re- 
search has  demonstrated  that  therapy  patients  go  through  more  pro- 
nounced and  varied  cycles  of  change  depending  on  the  kind  and  dura- 
tion of  psychotherapy. 

Another  important  consideration  in  evaluating  these  results  is  the 
possible  existence  of  extraneous  or  artificial  factors  pushing  the  find- 
ings in  a  falsely  positive  direction.  One  possibility  is  that  the  change 
in  pre-to-post  testing  could  be  the  result  of  a  regression  to  the  mean. 
If  the  second  tests  yielded  less  intense  or  less  extreme  scores  or  if  the 
initial  TATs  were  less  extreme  than  the  initial  self-descriptions,  then 
the  results  would  be  due  to  a  statistical  artifact.  An  examination  of  the 
data  revealed  that  neither  of  these  situations  existed.  At  this  point  it 
seems  possible  to  accept  the  hypothesis  that  there  is  a  general  and  sig- 
nificant tendency  for  private,  ''pre conscious^''  fantasy  material  {as 
tapped  by  the  TAT)  to  predict  the  kind  of  future  changes  in  con- 
scious self-perception. 

The  second  hypothesis  concerned  the  ability  of  the  TAT  to  predict 
the  amount  of  change.  It  will  be  recalled  that  the  third  discrepancy 
score  (d)  mentioned  above  reflects  the  linear  distance  on  the  diag- 
nostic grid  between  the  initial  self-description  score  and  the  TAT,  or 
the  distance  between  any  pre-versus-post  comparison.   If  the  second 


478 


APPENDICES 


hypothesis  is  correct,  the  greater  the  conflict  or  discrepancy  within 
the  pretest  personality,  the  greater  the  change  over  time.  Conversely, 
the  more  rigid  and  tightly  organized  the  pretest  personality  the 
smaller  the  change  to  be  expected.  Defined  operationally:  a  large  "d" 
score  between  Level  II  and  Level  III  in  the  pretesting  predicts  to  a 
large  "d"  score  between  the  pre-  and  posttesting  at  Level  II. 

This  comparison  was  made  for  the  psychotherapy  sample  and  the 
discussion  group  controls.  The  hypothesis  did  not  hold  for  the  therapy 
sample.  A  large  conflict  between  self-diagnosis  and  TAT  was  not 
related  to  a  large  pre-post  therapy  change.  It  did  hold  for  the  discus- 
sion group  sample.  As  will  be  seen  in  Table  53,  the  greater  the  dis- 
crepancy between  the  self-diagnosis  and  the  TAT  in  the  pretesting, 
the  more  likely  a  large  change  in  self -diagnosis  over  time.  For  the 
discussion  group  controls  it  can  be  said  that  structural  (interlevel) 
variability  predicts  to  temporal  variability. 

TABLE  53 

Chi-Square  Relating  the  Amount  of  Discrepancy  Between  Conscious  Self-Diag- 
nosis AND  TAT  Diagnosis  to  Amount  of  Temporal  Change  in  Self-Diagnosis  for 
81  Discussion  Group  Contpols 


Small  Discrepancy  (d) 

Between  Self-Diagnosis 

and  TAT  in  Initial  Testing 

Large  Discrepancy  (d) 

Between  Self-Diagnosis  and 

TAT  in  Initial  TesUng 

Small  Discrepancy  (d) 
Between  Pre  and  Post 
Self- Diagnosis 

27 

IS 

Large  Discrepancy  (d) 
Between  Pre  and  Post 
Self-Diagnosis 

16 

23 

4.40;  p  =  .05. 


The  question  as  to  why  the  hypothesis  holds  for  one  sample  and 
not  the  other  cannot  be  answered  satisfactorily  at  this  point.  It 
may  be  due  to  the  complexities  and  peculiarities  of  either  sample.  One 
answer  may  lie  in  the  fact  that  the  psychotherapy  sample  tends  to  be 
loaded  with  individuals  who  manifested  severe  interlevel  conflict  be- 
fore therapy  and  relatively  less  change  during  therapy.  The  typical 
therapy  patient,  as  described  in  another  publication  ( 1 ) ,  is  guilty,  de- 
pressed, and  passive  in  self-diagnosis  and  expresses  underlying  themes 
of  power  and  narcissism.  Although  some  change  does  occur  in  therapy 
patients,  this  intense  conflict  does  not  get  resolved  during  the  six- 
month  period  covered  by  the  pre-post  testing.  Further  research  into 


THE  LEVEL  III-TAT  479 

the  nature  and  sequence  of  change  in  psychotherapy  may  provide  the 
solution  to  this  issue. 

SuTnmary 

This  paper  has  presented  a  theory  of  and  measurement  method  for 
deahng  with  imaginative,  "preconscious"  behavior.  Some  functions 
of  imaginative  expressions  were  presented.  These  included  reduction 
of  anxiety  and  internal  tension,  maintenance  of  the  feeling  of  self- 
esteem  and  uniqueness,  the  time-binding  postponement  of  impulse. 
The  implication  of  these  theories  is  that  imaginative  productions  can 
be  used  by  the  psychologist  to  determine  the  amount  of  and  inter- 
personal sources  of  anxiety  and  to  predict  future  behavior.  The  Kaiser 
Foundation  method  for  rating  the  interpersonal  aspects  of  imagina- 
tive expressions  was  described  and  employed  for  illustration  purposes 
to  a  dream  of  Sigmund  Freud,  This  method  was  then  applied  to  two 
sets  of  data  to  test  the  hypothesis  that  fantasy  expressions  predict  the 
amount  and  kind  of  change  to  be  expected  in  future  conscious  self- 
descriptive  behavior.  The  results  tended  to  support  the  notion  that  the 
kind  of  structural  (interlevel)  variability  between  levels  in  pretesting 
predicts  the  kind  of  variabihty  to  occur  in  the  future.  The  hypothesis 
that  the  amount  of  structural  variability  is  related  to  the  amount  of 
temporal  variability  was  found  to  hold  for  one  sample  and  not  the 
second. 

References 

1.  Leary,  T.,  and  Joan  Harvey.  A  methodology  for  measuring  personality  changes 
in  psychotherapy.   /.  din.  Psychol.,  1956,  12,  No.  3,  123-32. 

2.  Murray,  H.  A.  Thematic  Apperception   Test.    Cambridge:    Harvard  University 
Press,  1943. 


4 


The  Interpersonal  Diagnostic  Report 


The  multilevel  pattern  of  scores  provided  by  the  interpersonal  system 
is  employed  to  make  diagnostic  and  prognostic  statements  about  pa- 
tients who  are  being  evaluated  for  psychotherapy.  The  scores  can  be 
converted  into  predictions  about  the  interpersonal  behavior  to  be  ex- 
pected in  the  subsequent  clinical  contacts. 

The  diagnostic  report  focuses  on  the  functional  aspects  of  the  pa- 
tient's personality  in  terms  of  five  areas: 

1.  Motivation  for  psychotherapy 

2.  "Preconscious"  conflicts  and  the  associated  defensive  processes 

3.  Summary  of  conscious  and  "preconscious"  identification  patterns 
and  the  predicted  transference  possibilities 

4.  Analysis  of  the  ego-structure,  ego  strength,  potential  psychotic 
tendencies 

5.  Prognosis  of  response  to  psychotherapy 

In  developing  these  clinical  implications  of  the  interpersonal  pro- 
file, we  have  been  influenced  by  the  concepts  of  Merton  Gill  et  al. 
In  the  book  The  Initial  Intervieiu  in  Psychiatric  Practice  (1)  Gill, 
Newman,  and  Redlich  outline  a  theory  and  technique  for  assessing 
the  variables  which  are  crucial  to  prognosis. 

A  sample  diagnostic  report  will  now  be  presented.  First  the  multi- 
level personality  pattern  and  the  family  relationships  (as  measured  by 
the  interpersonal  system)  are  described;  then  the  clinical  implications 
of  the  profile  are  listed. 

Multilevel  diagnosis  is  accomplished  most  efficiently  by  using  the 
"Record  Booklet  for  Interpersonal  Diagnosis  of  Personality."  A  copy 
of  this  printed  booklet  is  presented  in  Figure  61.  The  raw  scores, 
standard  scores,  diagnostic  profiles,  and  calculations  of  variability 
indices  for  the  sample  case  are  included  for  illustrative  purposes  in  this 
figure.  The  data  listed  in  the  diagnostic  booklet  will  allow  the  reader 
to  follow  the  step-by-step  derivation  of  the  multilevel  diagnosis.  The 

480 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT  481 

norms  for  converting  raw  scores  at  Levels  I-M,  II-C,  III-TAT 
(Hero),  and  III-TAT  (Other),  and  the  weighted  scores  for  variabil- 
ity indices  are  listed  in  Appendix  5. 

In  this  illustrative  case  the  patient  was  retested  after  psychotherapy. 
The  personality  profile  after  therapy  will  be  examined  to  check  on 
the  accuracy  of  the  original  predictions  and  to  illustrate  the  use  of 
the  interpersonal  system  in  measuring  personality  change.  The  post- 
therapy  (second  testing)  Level  I-M  and  II-C  diagnoses  of  this  sample 
patient  are  not  presented  in  the  pretherapy  diagnostic  booklet.  In 
order  to  illustrate  the  diagrammatic  measurement  of  change  in  per- 
sonality, Figure  62  presents  the  pretherapy  multilevel  pattern  and  the 
posttherapy  scores  at  Levels  I  and  II. 

Personality  Evaluation  of  Case  6618 

Multilevel  Personality  Profile.  This  40-year-old  man  mani- 
fests extreme  depression  and  helpless  dependence  in  his  symptoms  (see 
Figure  61).  His  MMPI  can  be  seen  as  a  plea  for  help  and  an  ex- 
pression of  weakness,  fear,  and  impotence. 

In  his  conscious  self-description  he  presents  a  similar  picture.  He 
tells  us  he  is  passive,  self-effacing,  and  timid.  He  denies  hostility  or 
strength. 

His  "preconscious"  expressions  (Level  III-T  Hero)  present  a  dif- 
ferent picture.  Moderate  strength  and  independence  appear  at  this 
underlying  level.  His  deeper  fantasy  descriptions  of  "others"  involve 
agreeability  and  nurturance. 

The  pattern  of  these  four  scores  (6618)  defines  a  conflict  between 
two  levels  of  overt  passivity  and  underlying  feelings  of  power.  The 
strength  which  is  consciously  denied  appears  close  to  the  surface  in 
his  imaginative  productions. 

Interpersonal  Diagnosis.  Dependent-masochistic  personality  with 
underlying  feelings  of  power  and  nurturance. 

Psychiatric  Diagnostic  Impression.   Phobic  personality. 

Family  Dynamics.  This  patient  sees  his  father  as  a  weak,  self- 
effacing  person.  His  mother  is  assigned  extraordinary  power  and  pres- 
tige (being  more  than  two  sigmas  above  the  mean  dominance  score). 
His  wife  is  seen  as  a  strong,  responsible  person. 

A  definite  compartmentalization  of  sex  roles  is  revealed.  Females 
are  strong  and  nurturant.  Males  are  weak  and  docile. 

His  ego  ideal  falls  very  close  to  his  pictures  of  wife  and  mother. 
This  indicates  he  idealizes  the  female  relatives  and  devaluates  his  own 
and  his  father's  weakness.  An  insecure  grasp  on  a  masculine  identifica- 
tion is  suggested. 


482 


APPENDICES 


■™5     •■■■ 

.:i^    I/I 


& 


i  1 

*      O 


o 
O 


H 


n  5  in  <o 

1      I     1     1 


e    (= 


s   % 


^ 


^11 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT 


483 


<*) 


a 


RSS9C      5   5?5«???5 


,1   1 1 1 1   111   1 1  i  i 


5:  M 


5 


S   ^  o    o 


g  I 


sill 


^  ^hM    I  ^ 


484 


APPENDICES 


a  c 

NO 

•-S 

^ 

'^ 

\ 

^ 

% 

^ 

1j 
P 

J 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

i^ 

^ 

S 

Q 

5} 

Ri 

R 

S5 

^ 

1 

J 

1 

>0 

1 

^ 

1 

§ 

Q 

1 

f 

Q 

^ 

\ 

^ 

.^ 

s 

H 

Cu 

1 

0 

ts 

0 

0 

d 

J 
0 

J 
0 

it 

?^ 

^: 

'tt 

n 

U3 

CO 

w 

CO 

S 

> 

u. 

a 

^ 

j 

0 
3 

3 

fa 
3 

CO 

3 

&  i" 


S  a. 


|J3 


. 

-^ 

^ 

T 

7~^^^ 

K^ 

^^ 

^* 

\ 

s 

X^ 

/  / 

C» 

\ 

\ 

'^V  ^^ 

^ 

^. 

1  ^ 

\"' 

A 

•  \ 

;^ 

*"■/         ^ 

s^ 

\ 

/\n 

\  0 

?7    ^  '^.^^ 

\ 

/^ 

/    ^ 

^N 

3 

ZJI-r 

'"'""I ' 

\ 

7 

■A    / 

/ 

/ 

/  <0 

v\ 

J 

/ 

w 

\. 

/// 

/ 

^C. 

J 

'% 

/ 

y^y 

/ 

^ 

' 

\                            "•  ^ 

K 

^ 

=^r"~~~- 

LW^ 

THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT 


485 


486 


APPENDICES 


— I- 

— r 

^ 

— T 

■~~" 

"■"  " 

T 

■^~ 

I 

o     - 

\ 

H       - 

-jL 

^ 

V 

Y 

J       - 

\    , 

A 

o     - 

V    ' 

1 

1 

^-r 

yjL 

y: 

' 

^-- 

4- 



* 

-*r 

r 

r 

i 

o     - 

/ 

Cd      - 

d       - 

/ 

1  : 

' 

o 

, 

' 

, 

T- 

[ 

u     - 

I    1 

\ 

, 

J     - 

o      . 

w 

u 

\ 

fe  : 

I 

I 

A 

1 

[ 

'_ 

^ 

t 

a     ' 

\l 

\ 

* 

3     Z 

\i 

\\ 

tt.     _ 

y 

\ 

9,-^r, 



1 

I 

___ 

•        '   ^ 

^^ssg 

si. 

^   > 

^ 

^ 

N 

>* 

<*- 

vo 

^ 

v^ 

vj 

5 

^ 

^ 

M^ 

i^   ^ 

> 

^ 

^ 

•* 

^ 

•^ 

■<» 

Mi 

VN 

a 
c  ^ 

Q 

^^ 

^ 

^ 

55 

s 

1 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

$ 

^1 

D          <i 

^   ^ 
1    V 

1 

\ 

? 

? 

^ 

1 

1 

1 

<i 

^ 

Q 

^ 

^ 

*  So            ** 

^ 

!8 

^ 
^ 

■5 

5 

^ 

^ 

^ 

^ 

05 

^ 

iilii: 

I      o 

2 

u. 

a 

1 

a 

u. 

a 

i 

s 

i 

u. 

n 

> 

> 

2 

> 

£ 

a 

> 

o 

a 

> 

> 

> 

2 

7. 

z 

O 

O 

o 

P 

c 

H 

p 

M 

■S 

< 

< 
O 

< 

z 

I 

H 

H 

P 

u 
o 

< 

Id 
U 

u 

1        S 

X         K 

Z 

ii 

U 

z 

u 

a 

ii 

z 

a 

2 
u 

i 

£ 

¥ 

: 

3 

u 

ii 
Ii 

td 

t 

0. 

told 

1.   DO 

8S 

u 
< 

[I. 

SIS 

OQ. 

It 

is 
is 

U  lb 

is 

i 

<  < 

ZN 

Is 

z 
9 

<•« 

z^ 

< 

(dji 

THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT 


487 


1 

\ 

' 

I 

\ 

1 

\ 

N  . 

\ 

^ 

1 

^ 

^ 

^ 

Q 

? 

N 

^ 

^ 

s 

u. 

5 

a 

5 
a 

X 

5 

lb 

B 

£ 

5 

a 

X 

5 

lb 

B 

i 

(b 

B 
i 

i 

5 

5 
lb 

B 

lb 

B 

S; 

B 

"3 
E 

a 

si 

i 
It 

1 
11 

\ 
si 

Si 

1 

ll 

i 

z 

1 

a 
ll 

z1 

0  * 

il 

u. 

2I 
11 

lb 

ll 
lb 

.1 

SI 

0 

idi 

El 

■J  i 

<2D, 

t! 

il 

i 

Q 

APPENDICES 


> 

• 

1 

^ 

!? 

^ 

§ 

§ 

2 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

\ 

1 

I 

o 
a 

1 

9 

• 

k 

1 

1 

i 
1 

"J 
1 

u 


a,      0.  V 


xl 


o    C 


«8 


to 

I 

I 


I 
I 


3n 


M       ••  o 


i    It    II 
!   si    ^1 

3"      !o  o      ^3 


u  B 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT  489 

His  own  view  of  self  is  far  removed  from  his  ego  ideal.  He  is, 
therefore,  dissatisfied  with  himself  and  desires  to  be  a  stronger  and 
mor'C  executive  person. 

His  "preconscious"  scores  fall  close  to  his  perceptions  of  mother 
and  wife.  This  means  that  there  is  a  "preconscious"  identification 
with  the  idealized  female  figures.  Consciously  he  is,  of  course,  more 
identified  with  the  weaker  father  and  disidentified  with  mother  and 
wife. 

It  will  be  noted  that  all  seven  interpersonal  scores  fall  on  the  right 
(conventional)  side  of  the  diagnostic  circle  and  none  on  the  left 
(rebellious)  side.  The  conflict  axis  lies  between  power  and  docility. 
This  indicates  that  any  "movement"  or  change  in  therapy  will  take 
place  vertically  on  the  right  side  of  the  diagnostic  circle. 

Clinical  Implications 

1 .  Motivation.  The  patient  is  initially  well  motivated  for  psycho- 
therapy. He  is  pushed  by  symptoms  of  anxiety  and  depression.  He  is 
consciously  dissatisfied  with  himself.  His  overt  security  operations 
are  dependent,  which  makes  it  easy  and  natural  for  him  to  play  the 
part  of  a  patient. 

2.  Preconscious  Conflicts  AND  Defense  Mechanisms.  A  conflict 
exists  between  overt  docility-masochism  and  "preconscious"  power. 
To  be  strong  is  to  be  feminine — i.e.,  like  mother  and  wife.  To  be 
masculine  (like  father)  means  to  be  passive.  Figure  61  includes  the 
variability  indices  of  interlevel  conflict  ^  and  verbal  summaries  of  these 
indices. 

It  will  be  seen  that  this  patient  represses  power  and  nurturance, 
that  he  is  consciously  identified  with  his  father  and  disidentified  with 
his  mother  and  his  wife,  and  that  he  is  "preconsciously"  identified  with 
these  female  relatives. 

*  The  variability  index  figures  cited  in  Figure  61  are  based  on  the  methodology  de- 
scribed in  Chapter  13.  The  indices  of  discrepancy  on  Dominance  and  Love  are  cal- 
culated separately  and  indicate  what  is  repressed  or  misperceived.  These  two  orthog- 
onal linear  indices  (vertical  and  horizontal)  are  the  sides  of  a  right  triangle  the 
hypotenuse  of  which  is  the  linear  discrepancy  distance.  The  hypotenuse  of  this 
triangle  is  the  variability  index  (d);  the  two  sides  of  the  triangle  indicate  whether 
dominance  (-f-)  or  passivity  (— )  and  love  (  +  )  or  hostility  (  — )  are  involved  in  the 
discrepancy.  The  weighted  scores  are  based  on  mathematical  procedures  outlined 
in  Chapter  13.  In  Appendix  5  will  be  found  a  complete  listing  of  the  weighted  scores 
for  every  possible  combination  of  scores  (i.e.,  interlevel  discrepancies  or  changes  in 
the  same  level  over  time). 

Figure  61  contains  a  column  for  entering  the  standard  scores  for  discrepancies. 
At  the  present  time  the  normative  studies  have  not  been  completed  and  tables  of 
standard  scores  are  not  available.  For  this  reason  this  column  is  left  blank.  The 
plotting  of  variability  indices  on  the  diagram  is  based  on  an  arbitrary  (and  tentative^ 
scaling  and  included  for  illustrative  purposes. 


490  APPENDICES 

3.  Predicted  Interpersonal  Reactions  to  Psychotherapy.  This 
patient  will  present  himself  as  docile  and  helpless.  He  will  exert  tre- 
mendous dependent  pressure  on  the  therapist,  attempting  to  provoke 
reassurance,  sympathy,  and  approval. 

The  underlying  feelings  of  power  will,  however,  lead  the  patient 
to  private  reservations  and  covert  feelings  of  stubborn  superiority. 
The  patient  pulls  for  sympathy  but  privately  does  not  want  to  be  seen 
as  passive.  A  power  struggle  could  develop  unless  the  therapist  avoids 
these  powerful  reflexes. 

The  underlying  power  motivation  suggests  that  the  patient  will 
not  remain  passive  and  will  try  to  end  therapy  through  becoming 
normal  and  responsible. 

The  sequence  to  be  expected  is  therefore:  docility  followed  by 
dominance. 

He  will  initially  attempt  to  provoke  maternal  strength  from  the 
therapist.  He  will  then  tend  (if  the  therapist  is  a  male)  to  assume 
power  and  to  derogate  and  master  the  therapist. 

4.  Prognosis.  The  prognosis  for  psychotherapy  is  in  general  posi- 
tive. There  are  no  indications  of  psychotic  processes.  The  under- 
lying feelings  of  responsibility  forecast  a  healthy  resolution  of  the 
symptomatic  pressures.  He  is  considered  a  good  candidate  for  brief 
symptom-oriented  psychotherapy. 

The  tangled  nature  of  the  sexual  identification  suggests  that  basic 
changes  in  the  personality  would  not  be  expected  short  of  psycho- 
analysis. He  is  therefore  considered  a  good  bet  only  for  brief  counsel- 
ing. He  will  probably  repress  out  of  therapy  and  move  towards  a 
closer  identification  with  his  family  members. 

He  cannot  be  expected  to  move  towards  masculine  independence 
or  rebellion  from  the  close  family  ties. 

Case  History 

The  cUnician  who  handled  this  case  provided  a  case  history  which 
was  written  without  knowledge  of  the  test  results. 

Clinical  Summary.  A  40-year-old  auto  mechanic,  self-referred 
with  complaints  of  insomnia,  hypertension,  dizzy  spells,  blurry  vision, 
prostate  trouble,  etc.  These  symptoms  had  begun  six  months  ago, 
when  a  friend  of  the  patient's  who  had  many  similar  symptoms  died  of 
a  heart  attack  following  a  prostate  operation.  The  patient's  physical 
condition  was  aggravated  by  business  developments  he  did  not  feel 
competent  to  handle.  The  patient's  home  situation  was  apparently 
satisfactory  ("wife  and  I  get  along  nicely"),  though  in  the  last  year 
or  so  he  hadn't  been  quite  the  man  he  used  to  be;  he  had  less  sexual 


THE  INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSTIC  REPORT 


491 


desire  than  he  used  to  have.  He  came  from  a  Jewish  family  who  lived 
in  a  Catholic,  anti-Semitic  neighborhood;  the  parents  fought  con- 
stantly. Patient  was  the  second  of  six  children.  He  had  worked  at 
various  jobs,  suffered  much  in  the  depression,  finally  took  up  auto 
mechanics  which  had  been  his  hobby. 

Impression:  very  nice  guy — long  history  of  hypochondriasis — feel- 
ings of  inadequacy  which  he  is  very  aware  of.   Passivity  problems. 


qV 


v\^" 


moc 


'%/, 


IH                7^ 

<''^X 

►                / 

HMo  / 
•      / 

3?Id  / 

/       nro 

m 

/ 

A  ^ 

•  nsp 

\_ 

JO 

r\^ 

/\  \ 

(S'C^ 

so 

99 

1       1^JMJ_J_1--^ 

[\     \   ♦ 

' 

V 

DA 

/ 

UFa 


'xsV^. 


% 


S^lr. 


^^FA 


I* 


£(^|^SOCHISTlC 


Figure   62.    Multilevel   Profile   of  Illustrative   Patient   Before   Psychotherapy  and 
Level  I-M  and  II-C  Scores  After  Psychotherapy. 


Key:  Before  therapy:  6618-1S8-1 
After  Therapy:  71 
I  =  Level  I-M  before  therapy 
S  =  Conscious  view  of  self 
H  =  Level  IIl-T  Hero 
O  =  Level  HI-T  Other 
M  =  Conscious  view  of  mother 


F  =  Conscious  view  of  father 
Sp  =  Conscious  view  of  spouse 
Id  =  Level  V-Ego  Ideal 

S=  Level  I-M  after  therapy 
=  Level  II-C— Conscious  view 
of  self  after  therapy 


4^2  APPENDICES 

Prior  to  the  second  interview  the  patient  called  to  say  his  private  M.D. 
had  kicked  him  out  since  he  was  getting  psychiatric  care,  he  had  no 
more  sleeping  pills,  and  what  to  do.  It  was  suggested  he  go  to  the 
medical  clinic  for  a  work  up,  which  he  did.  He  was  looking  and  feel- 
ing much  better  at  the  second  interview  after  having  stopped  medi- 
cation even  though  he  still  couldn't  sleep.  Therapy  was  discussed 
and  the  patient  showed  some  resistance  as  to  cost  and  daytime  hours 
but  accepted  it  as  inevitable.  He  was  seen  in  individual  therapy  fif- 
teen times.  Close-out  report  states  "patient  is  a  severe  phobic  .  .  .  ; 
motivation  was  always  a  problem — he  wanted  answers.  We  both 
agree  that  his  symptoms  are  the  price  he  pays  for  his  satisfactory  (to 
hun)  way  of  life."  There  was  some  symptomatic  improvement. 

Closing  Diagnosis.  Obsessive  neurosis  with  phobic  features. 

PosTTHERAPY  Test  Profile.  This  patient  was  administered  the  In- 
terpersonal Check  List  and  the  MMPI  after  fifteen  sessions  of  indi- 
vidual psychotherapy.  The  pre-  and  posttherapy  scores  are  presented 
in  Figure  62.  A  dramatic  symptomatic  improvement  was  recorded. 
His  Level  I-M  index  (\)  moves  in  the  direction  of  increased  power 
and  decreased  passivity.  He  ends  up  (at  Level  I)  with  the  diagnosis  of 
overconventional  (hysteric)  personality.  He  no  longer  admits  to  fears, 
worries,  and  depression  but  claims  conventional  adjustment. 

In  his  self-description  the  same  direction  of  change  occurs.  He 
sees  himself  after  therapy  as  a  confident,  executive  person.  This  re- 
vision, it  will  be  noted,  brings  him  into  a  much  closer  identification 
with  his  female  relatives.  He  is  utilizing  (at  this  level)  the  security 
operations  that  characterize  his  mother  and  wife.  The  permanence 
or  basic  efficiency  of  this  adjustment  may  be  questioned  but  it  is  clear 
that  considerable  symptomatic  relief  has  occurred  through  this  repres- 
sive process  and  that  the  patient  is  currently  happy  with  this  resolu- 
tion. 

Reference 

1.   Gill,  M.,  R.  Newman,  and  F.  Redlich.   The  initial  interview  in  psychiatric  prac- 
tice.  New  York:  International  Universities  Press,  1954. 


5 


NormSj  Conversion  Tables,  and  Weighted 
Scores  Used  in  Interpersonal  Diagnosis 


In  the  interpersonal  system  the  diagnosis  at  each  level  is  assigned 
automatically  and  objectively  by  locating  the  intersection  of  the  verti- 
cal (Dominance-Submission)  and  horizontal  (Love-Hostility)  indices 
on  the  diagnostic  grid.  This  procedure  is  described  in  Chapter  12. 
The  center  of  the  diagnostic  grid  is  determined  by  the  intersection  of 
the  means  of  the  horizontal  and  vertical  distribution  of  a  normative 
psychiatric  cHnic  admission  sample.  In  order  to  arrive  at  the  inter- 
personal diagnosis,  it  is  necessary  to  convert  the  raw  scores  (Domi- 
nance and  Love)  to  standard  scores  at  each  level. 

Table  54  presents  the  norms  used  for  converting  raw  scores  at 
Level  I-M  to  standard  scores.^  Table  SS  lists  the  norms  for  Level 
II-C.  Tables  56  and  57  present  the  norms  for  Level  III  TAT  (Hero) 
and  (Other)  respectively. 

The  system  for  measuring  discrepancies  between  levels  or  between 
two  tests  at  the  same  level  administered  at  different  times  is  presented 
in  Chapter  13.  Three  indices  of  variability  are  derived  from  each  pair 
of  diagnostic  codes  to  be  compared.  The  Dom  index  indicates  the 
amount  of  discrepancy  between  the  two  levels  on  the  dominance- 
submission  axis.  The  Lov  index  indicates  the  amount  of  discrepancy 
on  the  love-hostility  axis.  The  Dom  and  the  Lov  indices  thus  define 
the  kind  of  discrepancy.  The  amount  of  discrepancy  is  indicated  by 
the  "d"  value. 

The  weighted  scores  assigned  to  the  discrepancy  between  each 
pair  of  interlevel  scores  (or  between  scores  at  the  same  level  obtained 
at  different  times)  are  presented  in  Table  58.   It  will  be  noted  that 

*  Templates  and  diagnostic  grids  used  for  interpersonal  diagnosis  have  been  pub- 
lished by  the  Psychological  Consultation  Service,  1230  Queens  Road,  Berkeley  8, 
California. 

493 


TABLE  54 

Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard  Scores  at  Level 

I-M  (Standardized  on  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychology  Research 

Samples  B,  Cl-1,  Cl-2) 


Dom 

Standard 
Score 

Lov 

Dom 

Standard 
Score 

Lov 

447,  48,  49 

80 

+74.  75.  76 

445,  46 

79 

+72.  73 

-26,  27 

49 

-  8,    9,  10 

442,  43.  44 

78 

469,  70,  71 

-28,  29,  30 

48 

-11,  12 

440,41 

77 

4^6,  67,  68 

-31,  32 

47 

-13,  14.  15 

438,39 

76 

4^4,  65 

-33,  34,  35 

46 

-16,  17,  18 

+35,  36,  37 

75 

461,  62.  63 

-36,  37 

45 

-19,  20,  21 

433.34 

74 

458.  59.  60 

-38,  39,  40 

44 

-22,  23 

430,  31,  32 

73 

+55,  56,  57 

-41,  42 

43 

-24,  25,  26 

+28,  29 

72 

+53,  54 

-43,44 

42 

-27,  28,  29 

+25,  26,  27 

71 

+50,  51,  52 

-45,  46,  47 

41 

-30,  31 

+23.  24 

70 

447,  48,  49 

-48,  49 

40 

-32.  33,  34 

+21.  22 

«9 

445,46 

-50,  51,  52 

39 

-35,  36 

+18,  19,  20 

68 

442,  43,  44 

-53,  54 

38 

-37,  38,  39 

+16.  17 

67 

+39,  40,  41 

-55,  56 

37 

-40,  41,  42 

+13,  14,  15 

66 

+37,  38 

-57,  58,  59 

36 

-43,  44,  45 

+11,  12 

65 

+34.  35,  36 

-60.  61 

35 

-46,  47,  48 

+  9,  10 

64 

+31.  32,  33 

-62,  63,  64 

34 

-49,  50 

+  6,    7.    8 

63 

+28,  29,  30 

-65,  66 

33 

-51,  52.  S3 

+  4,    5 

62 

+26,  27 

-67,  68 

32 

-54,  55,  56 

+  1.    2,    3 

61 

+23,  24,  25 

-69,  70,  71 

31 

-57,  58 

-  1.    0 

60 

+20,  21,  22 

-72,  73 

30 

-59,  60,  61 

-  2,    3 

59 

+17,  18,  19 

-74,  75,  76 

29 

-62,  63.  64 

-  4.    5,    6 

58 

+15,  16 

-77,  78 

28 

-65,  66,  67 

-  7,    8 

57 

+12,  13,  14 

-79,  80.  81 

27 

-68,  69 

-  9,  10,  11 

56 

+  9,  10,  11 

-82,  83 

26 

-70,  71,  72 

-12.  13 

55 

+  7,    8 

-84,  85 

25 

-73,  74,  75 

-14.  15 

54 

+  4,    5,    6 

-86,  87,  88 

24 

-76,  77 

-16,  17.  18 

S3 

+  1,    2,    3 

-89,  90 

23 

-78,  79,  80 

-19,  20 

52 

0,-1,    2 

-91,  92,  93 

22 

-81,  82,  83 

-21,  22,  23 

51 

-  3,    4 

-94,95 

21 

-84,  85,  86 

-24,  25 

50 

-  5,    6,    7 

-96,  97,  98 

20 

-87,  88 

494 


TABLE  55 

Norms  for  CohJVERTmc  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard  Scores  at  Level 

II-C  (For  Use  with  Interpersonal  Check  List,  Form  4)    (Standardized  on  Kaiser 

Foundation  Research  Sample  G) 

Std.  Std.  Std.  StO. 

Dom       Score  Lov       Score  Dom      Score  Lov       Score 


+37.8 

+38.4 

102 

+37.0 

+37.7 

101 

+36.2 

+36.9 

100 

-  2.5 

-  3.2 

50 

+  1.2 

+  2.0 

SO 

+35.4 

+36.1 

99 

-  3.3 

-  4.0 

49 

+  0.3 

+  1.1 

49 

+34.6 

+35.3 

98 

-  4.1 

-  4.8 

48 

+  0.2 

-  0.6 

48 

+33.8 

+34.5 

97 

-'4.9 

-  5.6 

47 

-  0.7 

-  1.4 

47 

+33.0 

+33.7 

96 

-  5.7 

-  6.4 

46 

-  1.5 

-  2.3 

46 

+32.2 

+32.9 

95 

-  6.5 

-  7.2 

45 

-  2.4 

-  3.2 

45 

+31.5 

+32.1 

94 

-  7.3 

-  8.0 

44 

-  3.3 

-  4.1 

44 

+30.7 

+31.4 

93 

-  8.1 

-  8.7 

43 

-  4.2 

-  5.0 

43 

+29.9 

+30.6 

92 

-  8.8 

-  9.5 

42 

-  5.1 

-  5.9 

42 

+29.1 

+29.8 

91 

+37.6 

+38.4 

91 

-  9.6 

-10.3 

41 

-  6.0 

-  6.8 

41 

+28.3 

+29.0 

90 

+36.7 

+37.5 

90 

-10.4 

-11.1 

40 

-  6.9 

-  7.7 

40 

+27.5 

+28.2 

89 

+35.9 

+36.6 

89 

-11.2 

-11.9 

39 

-  7.8 

-  8.3 

39 

+26.7 

+27.4 

88 

+35.0 

+35.8 

88 

-12.0 

-12.7 

38 

-  8.6 

-  9.4 

38 

+25.9 

+26.6 

87 

+34.1 

+34.9 

87 

-12.8 

-13.5 

37 

-  9.5 

-10.3 

37 

+25.2 

+25.8 

U 

+33.2 

+34.0 

86 

-13.6 

-14.3 

36 

-10.4 

-11.2 

36 

+24.4 

+25.1 

85 

+32.3 

+33.1 

85 

-14.4 

-15.0 

35 

-11.3 

-12.1 

35 

+23.6 

+24.3 

84 

+31.4 

+32.2 

84 

-15.1 

-15.8 

34 

-12.2 

-13.0 

34 

+22.8 

+23.5 

83 

+30.5 

+31.3 

83 

-15.9 

-16.6 

33 

-13.1 

-13.9 

33 

+22.0 

+22.7 

82 

+29.6 

+30.4 

82 

-16.7 

-17.4 

32 

-14.0 

-14.8 

32 

+21.2 

+21.9 

81 

+28.8 

+29.5 

81 

-17.5 

-18.2 

31 

-14.9 

-15.6 

31 

+20.4 

+21.1 

80 

+27.9 

+28.7 

80 

-18.3 

-19.0 

30 

-15.7 

-16.5 

30 

+19.6 

+20.3 

79 

+27.0 

+27.8 

79 

-19.1 

-19.8 

29 

-16.6 

-17.4 

29 

+18.9 

+19  5 

78 

+26.1 

+26.9 

78 

-19.9 

-20.6 

28 

-17.5 

-18.3 

28 

+18.1 

+18  8 

77 

+25.2 

+26.0 

77 

-20.7 

-21.3 

27 

-18.4 

-19.2 

27 

+17.3 

+18.0 

76 

+24.3 

+25.1 

76 

-21.4 

-22.1 

26 

-19.3 

-20.1 

26 

+16.5 

+17.2 

75 

+23.4 

+24.2 

75 

-22.2 

-22.9 

25 

-20.2 

-21.0 

25 

+15.7 

+16.4 

74 

+22.5 

+23.3 

74 

-23.0 

-23.7 

24 

-21.1 

-21.9 

24 

+14.9 

+15.6 

73 

+21.7 

+22.4 

73 

-23.8 

-24.5 

23 

-22.0 

-22.8 

23 

+14.1 

+14.8 

72 

+20.8 

+21.6 

72 

-24.6 

-25.3 

22 

-22.9 

-23.6 

22 

+13.3 

+14.0 

71 

+19.9 

+20.7 

71 

-25.4 

-26.1 

21 

-23.7 

-24.5 

21 

+12.5 

+13.2 

70 

+19.0 

+19.8 

70 

-26.2 

-26.9 

20 

-24.6 

-25.4 

20 

+11.7 

+12.4 

69 

+18.1 

+18.9 

69 

-27.0 

-27.6 

19 

-25.5 

-26.3 

19 

+11.0 

+11.6 

68 

+17.2 

+18.0 

68 

-27.7 

-28.4 

18 

-26.4 

-27.2 

18 

+10.2 

+10.9 

67 

+16.3 

+17.1 

67 

-28.5 

-29.2 

17 

-27.3 

-28.1 

17 

+  9.4 

+10.1 

66 

+15.4 

+16.2 

66 

-29.3 

-30.0 

16 

-28.2 

-29.0 

16 

+  8.6 

+  9.3 

65 

+14.5 

+15.3 

65 

-30.1 

-30.8 

IS 

-29.1 

-29.9 

15 

+  7.8 

+  8.5 

64 

+13.7 

+14.4 

64 

-30.9 

-31.6 

14 

-30.0 

-30  7 

14 

+  7.0 

+  7.7 

63 

+12.8 

+13.6 

63 

-31.7 

-32.4 

13 

-30.8 

-31.6 

13 

+  6.2 

+  6.9 

62 

+11.9 

+12.7 

62 

-32.5 

-33.2 

12 

-31.7 

-32.5 

12 

+  5.4 

+  6.1 

61 

+11.0 

+11.8 

61 

-33.3 

-33.9 

11 

-32.6 

-33  4 

11 

+  4.7 

+  5.3 

60 

+10.1 

+10.9 

60 

-34.0 

-34.7 

10 

-33.5 

-34.3 

10 

+  3.9 

+  4.6 

59 

+  9.2 

+10.0 

59 

-34.8 

-35.5 

9 

-34.4 

-35.2 

9 

+  3.1 

+  3.8 

58 

+  8.3 

+  9.1 

58 

-35.6 

-36.3 

8 

-35.3 

-36.1 

8 

+  2.3 

+  3.0 

57 

+  7.4 

+  8.2 

57 

-36.4 

-37.1 

7 

-36.2 

-37.0 

7 

+  1.5 

+  2.2 

56 

+  6.6 

+  7.3 

56 

-37.2 

-37.9 

6 

-37.1 

-37.9 

6 

+  0.7 

+  1.4 

55 

+  5.7 

+  6.5 

55 

-38.0 

-38.4 

5 

-38.0 

-38.4 

5 

-  0.1 

+  0.6 

54 

+  4.8 

+  5.6 

54 

-  0.2 

-  0.9 

53 

+  3.9 

+  4.7 

53 

-  1.0 

-   1.6 

52 

+  3.0 

+  3.8 

52 

-  1.7 

-  2.4 

51 

+  2.1 

+  2.9 

51 

495 


TABLE  56 

Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard  Scores  at  Level 

UI-TAT  (Hero)   (Standardized  on  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychology  Research 

Sample  100) 


Std. 

Std. 

Std. 

Sic 

Dom    Score 

Lov       Score 

Dom      Score 

Lov 

Score 

+7.7    +7.9 

90 

+11.9 

+12.1 

90 

+7.4    +7.6 

89 

+11.6 

+11.8 

89 

- 

5.9 

-  6.1 

49 

-  0.1 

-  0.3 

49 

+7.0    +7.3 

88 

+11.3 

+11.5 

88 

. 

6.2 

-  6.5 

48 

-  0.4 

-  0.6 

48 

+6  7    +6.9 

87 

+11.0 

+11.2 

87 

. 

6.6 

-68 

47 

-  0.7 

-  0.9 

47 

+63    +6.6 

8< 

+10.7 

+10.9 

86 

- 

6.9 

-  7.1 

46 

-  LO 

-  1.2 

46 

+€.0    +6.2 

85 

+10.4 

+10.6 

85 

_ 

7.2 

-  7.5 

45 

-  1.3 

-   1.5 

45 

+5.7    +5.9 

84 

+10.1 

+10.3 

84 

- 

7.6 

-   7.8 

44 

-  1.6 

-  1.8 

44 

+5.3    +5.6 

83 

+  98 

+10.0 

83 

- 

7.9 

-  8.1 

43 

-  1.9 

-  2.1 

43 

+5.0     +5.2 

82 

+  9.5 

+  9.7 

82 

- 

8.2 

-  8.5 

42 

-  2.2 

-  2.4 

42 

+4.7    +4.9 

81 

+  9.2 

+  9.4 

81 

- 

8.6 

-88 

41 

-  2.5 

-  2.7 

41 

44.3    +4.6 

80 

+  8.9 

+  9.1 

80 

- 

8.9 

-  9.2 

40 

-  2.8 

-  3.0 

40 

+4.0 

+4.2 

79 

+  8.6 

+  8.8 

79 

-  9.3 

-  9.5 

39 

-  3.1 

-  3.3 

39 

+3.6 

+3.9 

78 

+  8.3 

+  8.5 

78 

-  9.6 

-  9.8 

38 

-  3.4 

-  3.6 

38 

+3.3 

+3.5 

77 

+  8.0 

+  8.2 

77 

-  9.9 

-10.2 

37 

-  3.7 

-  3.9 

37 

+3.0 

+3.2 

76 

+  7.7 

+  7.9 

76 

-10.3 

-10.5 

36 

-  4.0 

-  4.2 

36 

+2.6 

+2.9 

75 

+  7.4 

+  7.6 

75 

-10.6 

-10.8 

35 

-  4.3 

-  4.5 

35 

+2.3 

+2.5 

74 

+  7.1 

+  7.3 

74 

-10.9 

-11.2 

34 

-  4.6 

-  4.8 

34 

+2.0 

+2.2 

73 

+  6.8 

+  7.0 

73 

-11.3 

-11.5 

33 

-  4.9 

-  5.1 

33 

+1.6 

+1.9 

72 

+  6.5 

+  6.7 

72 

-11.6 

-11.9 

32 

-  5.2 

-  5.4 

32 

+1.3 

+1.5 

71 

+  6.2 

+  6.4 

71 

-12.0 

-12.2 

31 

-  5.5 

-  5.7 

31 

+1.0 

+1.2 

70 

+  6.0 

+  6.1 

70 

-12.3 

-12.5 

30 

-  5.8 

-  6.0 

30 

+0.6 

+0.9 

69 

+  5.7 

+  5.9 

69 

-12.6 

-12.9 

29 

-  6.1 

-  6.3 

29 

+0.3 

+0.5 

68 

+  5.4 

+  5.6 

68 

-13.0 

-13.2 

28 

-  6.4 

-  6.6 

28 

-0.1 

+0.2 

67 

+  5.1 

+  5.3 

67 

-13.3 

-13.5 

27 

-  6.7 

-  6.9 

27 

-0.2 

-0.4 

66 

+  4.8 

+  5.0 

66 

-13.6 

-13.9 

26 

-  7.0 

-  7.2 

26 

-0.5 

-0.7 

65 

+  4.5 

+  4.7 

65 

-14.0 

-14.2 

25 

-  7.3 

-  7.5 

25 

-0.8 

-1.1 

64 

+  4.2 

+  4.4 

64 

-14.3 

-14.6 

24 

-  7.6 

-  7.8 

24 

-1.2 

-1.4 

63 

+  3.9 

+  4.1 

63 

-14.7 

-14.9 

23 

-  7.9 

-  8.1 

23 

-1.5 

-1.7 

62 

+  3.6 

+  3.8 

62 

-15.0 

-15.2 

22 

-  8.2 

-  8.4 

22 

-1.8 

-2.1 

61 

+  3.3 

+  3.5 

61 

-15.3 

-15.6 

21 

-  8.5 

-  8.7 

21 

-2.2 

-2.4 

60 

+  3.0 

+  3.2 

60 

-15.7 

-15.9 

20 

-  8.8 

-  8.9 

20 

-2.5 

-2.8 

59 

+  2.7 

+  2.9 

59 

-2.9 

-3.1 

58 

+  2.4 

+  2.6 

58 

-3.2 

-3.4 

57 

+  2.1 

+  2.3 

57 

-3.5 

-3.8 

56 

+  1.8 

+  2.0 

56 

-3.9 

-4.1 

55 

+  1.5 

+  1.7 

55 

-4.2 

-4.4 

54 

+  1.2 

+  1.4 

54 

-4.5 

-4.8 

53 

+  0.9 

+  1.1 

53 

-4.9 

-5.1 

52 

+  0.6 

+  0.8 

52 

-5.2 

-5.5 

51 

+  0.3 

+  0.5 

51 

-5.6 

-5  8 

50 

0.0 

+  0.2 

50 

-16.0 

-16.2 

19 

-16.3 

-16.6 

18 

-16.7 

-16.9 

17 

-17.0 

-17.2 

16 

-17.3 

-17.6 

15 

-17.7 

-17.9 

14 

-18.0 

-18.3 

13 

-18.4 

-18.6 

12 

-18.5 

-18.9 

11 

-19  0 

-19.3 

10 

-  9.0    -  9.2     19 


9.3 

-  9.5 

18 

•  9.6 

-  9.8 

17 

■  9.9 

-10.1 

16 

•10.2 

-10  4 

15 

•10.5 

-10.7 

14 

•10  8 

-11.0 

13 

•11.1 

-113 

12 

•11.4 

-11.6 

11 

■11.7 

-119 

10 

496 


TABLE  57 

Norms  for  Converting  Raw  Scores  (Dom  and  Lov)  to  Standard  Scores  at  Level 

III-TAT   (Other)    (Standardized  on  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychology  Research 

Sample  1(X)) 

Std.  Std.  Std.  Std. 

Lov        Score  Dom     Score  Lov        Score 


Dom     Score 

49.5 

+9.7 

90 

+9.3 

+9.4 

89 

+9.0 

+9.2 

88 

+8.8 

+8.9 

87 

+8.6 

+8.7 

86 

+8.3 

+8.5 

85 

+8.1 

+8.2 

84 

+7.8 

+8.0 

83 

+7.6 

+7.7 

82 

+7.4 

+7.5 

81 

+6.9 

+7.0 

79 

+€.6 

+6.8 

78 

+€.4 

+6.5 

77 

+6.2 

+6.3 

76 

+5.9 

+6.1 

75 

+5.7 

+5.8 

74 

+5.4 

+5.6 

73 

+5.2 

+5.3 

72 

+5.0 

+5.1 

71 

+4.7 

+4,9 

70 

+4.5 

+4.6 

«9 

+4.2 

+4.4 

68 

+4.0 

+4.1 

67 

+3.7 

+3,9 

66 

+3.5 

+3.6 

65 

+3.3 

+3.4 

64 

+3.0 

+3.2 

63 

+2.8 

+2.9 

62 

+2.5 

+2.7 

61 

+2.3 

+2.4 

60 

+2.1 

+2.2 

59 

+1.8 

+2.0 

58 

+1.6 

+1.7 

57 

+1.3 

+1.5 

56 

+1.1 

+1  2 

55 

+0.9 

+1.0 

54 

+0.6 

+0.8 

53 

+0.4 

+05 

52 

+0.1 

+0.3 

51 

-0  1 

0  0 

50 

+10.0 

+10.2 

90 

+  9.8 

+  9.9 

89 

+  9.5 

+  9.7 

88 

+  9.2 

+  9.4 

87 

+  9.0 

+  9.1 

86 

+  8.7 

+  8.9 

85 

+  8.5 

+  8.6 

84 

+  8.2 

+  8.4 

83 

+  7.9 

+  8.1 

82 

+  7.7 

+  7.8 

81 

+  7.2 

+  7.3 

79 

+  6.9 

+  7.1 

78 

+  6.7 

+  6.8 

77 

+  6.4 

+  6.6 

76 

+  6.1 

+  6.3 

75 

+  5.9 

+  6.0 

74 

+  5.6 

+  5.8 

73 

+  5.4 

+  5.5 

72 

+  5.1 

+  5.3 

71 

+  4.8 

+  5.0 

70 

+  4.6    +  4.7    69 


+  4.3 

+  4.5 

68 

+  4.1 

+  4.2 

67 

+  3.8 

+  4.0 

66 

+  3.5 

+  3.7 

65 

+  3.3 

+  3.4 

64 

+  3.0 

+  3.2 

63 

+  2.8 

+  2.9 

62 

+  2.5 

+  2.7 

61 

+  2.3 

+  2.4 

60 

+  2.0 

+  2.2 

59 

+  1.7 

+  1.9 

58 

+  1.5 

+  1.6 

57 

+  1.2 

+  1.4 

56 

+  1.0 

+  1.1 

55 

+  0.7 

+  0.9 

54 

+  0.4 

+  0.6 

53 

+  0.2 

+  0.3 

52 

-  0.1 

+  0.1 

51 

-  0.2 

-  0.3 

50 

-0.5 

-0.4 

49 

-0.5 

-0.6 

48 

-0.7 

-0.8 

47 

-0.9 

-1.1 

46 

-1.2 

-1.3 

45 

-1.4 

-1.6 

44 

-1.7 

-1.8 

43 

-1.9 

-2.0 

42 

-2.1 

-2.3 

41 

+7.1     +7.3    80  +  7.4     +  7.6    80  -2.4    -2.5    40 


-2.6 

-2.8 

39 

-2.9 

-3.0 

38 

-3.1 

-3.2 

37 

-3.3 

-3.5 

36 

-3.6 

-3.7 

35 

-3.8 

-4.0 

34 

-4.1 

-4.2 

33 

-4.3 

-4.4 

32 

-4.5 

-4.7 

31 

-4,8 

-4.9 

30 

-5.0 

-5.2 

29 

-5.3 

-5.4 

28 

-5.5 

-5.7 

27 

-5.8 

-5.9 

26 

-6,0 

-6.1 

25 

-6.2 

-6.4 

24 

-6.5 

-6.6 

23 

-6.7 

-6.9 

22 

-7.0 

-7.1 

21 

-7.2 

-7.3 

20 

-7.4 

-7.6 

19 

-7.7 

-7.8 

18 

-7.9 

-8.1 

17 

-8.2 

-8.3 

16 

-8.4 

-8.5 

15 

-8.6 

-8.8 

14 

-8.9 

-9.0 

13 

-9.1 

-9.3 

12 

-9.4 

-9.5 

11 

-9  6 

-9.8 

10 

-  0.4 

- 

0.6 

49 

-  0.7 

_ 

0.9 

48 

-  0.1 

- 

1.1 

47 

-  L2 

- 

1.4 

46 

-   1.5 

- 

1.6 

45 

-   1.7 

_ 

1.9 

44 

-  2.0 

- 

2.2 

43 

-  2.3 

- 

2.4 

42 

-  2.5 

- 

2.7 

41 

-  2.8 

- 

2.9 

40 

-  3.0 

3.2 

39 

-  3.3 

- 

3.4 

38 

-  3.5 

- 

3.7 

37 

-  3.8 

- 

4.0 

36 

-  4.1 

- 

4.2 

35 

-  4.3 

- 

4.5 

34 

-  4.6 

- 

4.7 

33 

-  4.8 

- 

5.0 

32 

-51 

- 

5.3 

31 

-  5.4    -  5.5    30 


-  5.6 

-  5.8 

29 

-  5.9 

-  6.0 

28 

-  6.1 

-  6.3 

27 

-  6.4 

-  6.6 

26 

-  6.7 

-  6.8 

25 

-  6.9 

-  7.1 

24 

-  7.2 

-  7.3 

23 

■   7.4 

-  7.6 

22 

-  7.7 

-  7.9 

21 

-  8.0 

-  8.1 

20 

■  8.2 

-  8.4 

•  8.5 

-  8.6 

•  8.7 

-  8.9 

■  9.0 

-  9.1 

•  9.2 

-  9.4 

•  9.5 

-  9.7 

•  9.8 

-  9.9 

10.0 

-10.2 

10.3 

-10.4 

10.5 

-10.7 

497 


TABLE  58 

Weighted  Scores   for   Measuring   Discrepancy  Between  Two   Diagnostic   Codes 
Indicating  Kind  and  Amount  of  Difference  Between  Levels  or  Tests 


Weighted  Scores  Used  to  Compare 
Diagnostic  Codes  of  Extreme 
Intenfilty  (Both  Roman  Codes) 


Weighted  Scores  Used  to  Compare 

Diagnostic  Codes  of  Moderate 

Intensity  (Both  Italic  Codes) 


o2 

v3 

"^^ 

«S 

111 

III 

i 

ll 

266 

2^1 

til 

D 

L 

d 

D 

L 

d 

D 

L 

d 

D 

L 

d 

11 

00 

00 

00 

21 

+    9 

+  43 

44 

11 

00 

00 

00 

27 

+  5 

+25 

26 

12 

-     9 

-  43 

44 

22 

00 

00 

00 

u 

-  5 

-25 

26 

22 

00 

00 

00 

13 

-  45 

-  67 

81 

23 

-  36 

-  24 

44 

13 

-27 

-39 

48 

23 

-22 

-14 

26 

14 

-  88 

-  58 

105 

24 

-  79 

-  15 

81 

14 

-52 

-34 

62 

24 

-47 

-  9 

48 

15 

-112 

-  22 

114 

25 

-103 

+  21 

105 

15 

-66 

-12 

68 

25 

-61 

+13 

62 

16 

-103 

+  21 

105 

26 

-  94 

+  64 

114 

76 

-61 

+13 

62 

26 

-56 

+38 

68 

17 

-  67 

+  45 

81 

27 

-  58 

+  88 

105 

17 

-39 

+27 

48 

27 

-34 

+52 

62 

18 

-  24 

+  36 

44 

28 

-  15 

+  79 

81 

18 

-14 

+22 

26 

28 

-  9 

+47 

48 

31 

+  45 

+  67 

81 

41 

+  88 

+  58 

105 

31 

+27 

+39 

48 

47 

+52 

+34 

62 

32 

+  36 

+  24 

44 

42 

+  79 

+  15 

81 

32 

+22 

+14 

26 

42 

447 

+  9 

48 

33 

00 

00 

00 

43 

+  43 

-     9 

44 

33 

00 

00 

00 

43 

+25 

-  5 

26 

34 

-  43 

+     9 

44 

44 

00 

00 

00 

34 

-25 

+  5 

26 

44 

00 

00 

00 

35 

-  67 

+  45 

81 

45 

-  24 

+  36 

44 

35 

-39 

+27 

48 

45 

-14 

+22 

26 

36 

-  58 

+  88 

105 

46 

-  15 

+  79 

81 

36 

-34 

+52 

62 

46 

-  9 

+47 

48 

37 

-  22 

+112 

114 

47 

+  21 

+103 

105 

37 

-12 

+€6 

68 

47 

+13 

+61 

62 

38 

+  21 

+103 

105 

48 

+  64 

+  94 

114 

38 

+13 

+61 

62 

48 

+38 

+56 

68 

51 

+112 

+  22 

114 

61 

+103 

-  21 

105 

51 

+66 

+12 

68 

61 

+61 

-13 

62 

52 

+103 

-  21 

105 

62 

+  94 

-  64 

114 

52 

+61 

-13 

62 

62 

+56 

-38 

68 

53 

+  67 

-  45 

81 

63 

+  58 

-  88 

105 

53 

+39 

-27 

48 

63 

+34 

-52 

62 

54 

+  24 

-  36 

44 

64 

+  15 

-  79 

81 

54 

+14 

-22 

26 

64 

+  9 

-47 

48 

55 

00 

00 

00 

65 

-     9 

-  43 

44 

55 

00 

00 

00 

65 

-  5 

-25 

26 

56 

+     9 

+  43 

44 

66 

00 

00 

00 

56 

+  5 

+25 

26 

66 

00 

00 

GO 

57 

+  45 

+  67 

81 

67 

+  36 

+  24 

44 

57 

+27 

+39 

48 

67 

+22 

+14 

26 

58 

+  88 

+  58 

105 

68 

+  79 

+  15 

81 

58 

+52 

+34 

62 

68 

+47 

+  9 

48 

71 

+  67 

-  45 

81 

81 

+  24 

-  36 

44 

71 

+39 

-27 

48 

81 

+14 

-22 

26 

72 

+  58 

-  88 

105 

82 

+  15 

-  79 

81 

72 

+34 

-52 

62 

82 

+  9 

-47 

48 

73 

+  22 

-112 

114 

83 

-  21 

-103 

105 

73 

+12 

-66 

68 

83 

-13 

-61 

62 

74 

-  21 

-103 

105 

84 

-  64 

-  94 

114 

74 

-13 

-61 

62 

84 

-38 

-56 

68 

75 

-  45 

-  67 

81 

85 

-  88 

-  58 

105 

75 

-27 

-39 

48 

85 

-52 

-34 

62 

76 

-  36 

-24 

44 

86 

-  79 

-  15 

81 

76 

-22 

-14 

26 

86 

-47 

-  9 

48 

77 

00 

00 

00 

87 

-  43 

+    9 

44 

77 

00 

00 

00 

87 

-25 

+  5 

26 

78 

+  43 

-     9 

44 

88 

00 

00 

00 

78 

+25 

-  5 

26 

88 

00 

00 

00 

498 


TABLE  58  (.Continued) 

Weighted  Scores  Used  To  Compare  Diagnostic  Codes  Where  One  Diagnostic  Code 
Is  of  Extreme  and  the  Other  Is  of  Moderate  Intensity 

'%\  h         h  h 

%i  \i         ii  \i 

II  II  if  If 

Qo  QCD  aa  oa 


IT  -23  -  5  23 

12  -28  -30  41 

13  -50  -44  66 

14  -75  -39  84 


15  -89  -17  91 

16  -84  +  8  84 

17  -62  +22  66 

18  -37  +17  41 


3T  +22  +62  66 

32  +17  +37  41 

33  -  5  +23  23 

34  -30  +28  41 


27 

-14 

+38 

41 

n 

+23 

+  5 

23 

21 

+28 

+30 

41 

22 

-19 

+13 

23 

72 

+14 

-38 

41 

22 

+19 

-13 

23 

23 

-41 

-  1 

41 

73 

-22 

-62 

66 

23 

-17 

-37 

41 

24 

-66 

+  4 

66 

74 

-65 

-53 

84 

24 

-60 

-28 

66 

25 

-80 

+26 

84 

75 

-89 

-17 

91 

25 

-84 

+  8 

84 

26 

-75 

+51 

91 

76 

-80 

+26 

84 

26 

-75 

+51 

91 

27 

-53 

+65 

84 

77 

-44 

+50 

66 

27 

-39 

+75 

84 

28 

-28 

+€0 

66 

78 

-  1 

+41 

41 

28 

+  4 

+66 

66 

41 

+65 

+53 

84 

31 

+50 

+44 

66 

41 

+75 

+39 

84 

42 

+60 

+28 

66 

32 

+41 

+  1 

41 

42 

+66 

-  4 

66 

43 

+38 

+14 

41 

33 

+  5 

-23 

23 

43 

+30 

-28 

41 

44 

+13 

+19 

23 

34 

-38 

-14 

41 

44 

-13 

-19 

23 

35  -44  +50  66  45-1  +41  41  35  -62  +22  66  45       -37  +17  41 

36  -39  +75  84  46+4  +66  66  36  -53  +65  84  46       -28  +60  66 

37  -17  +89  91  47       +26  +80  84  37  -17  +89  91  47+8  +«4  84 

38  +8  +84  84  48       +51  +75  91  38  +26  +80  84  48       +51  +75  91 


57  +89  +17  91  67  +80  -26  84  51  +89  +17  91  61  +84  -  8  84 

52  +84  -  8  84  62  +75  -51  91  52  +80  -26  84  62  +75  -51  91 

53  +62  -22  66  63  +53  -65  84  53  +44  -50  66  63  +39  -75  84 

54  +37  -17  41  64  +28  -60  66  54+1  -41  41  54-4  -66  66 

55  +23  +  5  23  65  +14  -38  41  55  -23  -  5  23  65  -28  -30  41 

56  +28  +30  41  66  +19  -13  23  56  -14  +38  41  66  -19  +13  23 

57  +50  +44  66  67  +41+1  41  57  +22  +62  66  67  +17  +37  41 

58  +75  +39  84  68  +66  -  4  66  58  +«5  +53  84  68  +60  +28  66 


77  +44  -50  66  87       +1  -41  41  71  +62  -22  66  81       +37  -17  41 

72  +39  -75  84  82-4  -66  66  72  +53  -65  84  82       +28  -60  6<> 

73  +17  -89  91  83       -26  -80  84  73  +17  -89  91  83-8  -84  84 

74  -  8  -84  84  84       -51  -75  91  74  -26  -80  84  84      -51  -75  91 


75 

-22 

-62 

66 

85 

-65 

-53 

84 

75 

-50 

-44 

66 

85 

-75  -39 

84 

76 

-17 

-37 

41 

86 

-60 

-28 

66 

76 

-41 

-  1 

41 

86 

-66  +  4 

66 

77 

+  5 

-23 

23 

87 

-38 

-14 

41 

77 

-  5 

+23 

23 

87 

-30  +28 

41 

78 

+30 

-28 

41 

88 

-13 

-19 

23 

78 

+38 

+14 

41 

88 

+13  +19 

23 

499 


^oo  APPENDICES 

Table  58  is  divided  into  two  sections.  The  first  part  lists  the  weighted 
scores  used  to  compare  diagnostic  codes  of  the  same  intensity — i.e., 
both  extreme  (indicated  by  arabic  numerals)  or  both  moderate  (in- 
dicated by  italicized  numerals) .  If  the  diagnosis  at  one  level  is  intense 
and  the  diagnosis  at  the  other  level  moderate  (or  vice  versa)  the  sec- 
ond section  of  Table  58  is  used. 


Index  of  Names 


Alexander,  F.,  402 
Alvarez,  W.  C,  402 
Aristotle,  32 
Asch,  P.,  238 

Bales,  R.  F.,  102,  103,  131 
Bateson,  G^  21,  32 
Bendix,  R.,  93,  163,  191 
Bion,  W.  R.,  102,  103,  131 
Blum,  G.  S.,  87,  191 
Bolles,  Marjorie,  314 
Bolomey,  A.  A.,  375 
Breuer,  J.,  154 
Bridgman,  P.  W.,  47 
Brunswick,  Egon,  121,  131,  252 

Carmichael,  H.  T.,  238 

Carnap,  R.,  35 

Cassirer,  E.,  99,  131 

Charcot,  J.  M.,  310 

Coffey,  H.  S.,  240,  291,  436,  454 

Cutler,  Richard,  144,  153 

Darwin,  Charles,  102 
Doering,  C.  R.,  238 
Dollard,  J.,  102 

Elkin,  F.,  238 

Erikson,  Erik  H.,  7,  10,  11,  12,  13,  16,  20, 
26,  127 

Fenichel,  O.,  16,  297,  302,  314,  328,  331 
Frank,  Jerome,  275,  276,  281,  329,  331,  337, 

338 
Freedman,  M.,  240 
Freud,  Sigmund,  3,  7,  8,  11,  12,  19,  20,  22, 

37,  40,  71,  72,  75,  76,  87,  154,  165,  171, 

172,  173,  191,  302,  328,  331 
Fromm,  Erich,  7,  8,  10,  12,  16,  22,  23,  24, 

26,  27,  30,  32,  102,  231,  337 

Gilbert,  G.  M.,  184,  191 
Gill,  M.,  492 
Grotjohn,  M.,  284,  291 

Harris,  Robert  E.,  208,  236,  238 


Hartman,  H.  R.,  402 

Harvey,  Joan,  257  ff .,  478;  see  also  La- 
Forge,  Joan  Harvey 

Haskell,  Edward  F.,  74 

Heath,  Robert  G.,  3 

Hecht,  Shirley,  291 

Henderson,  Joseph,  22 

Henry,  William  E.,  198,  199 

Hippocrates,  71 

Homey,  Karen,  7,  8,  10,  12,  16,  22,  26, 
30,  102,  231,  337 

Iflund,  Boris,  87,  191,  198,  199 

Joyce,  James,  271 
Jung,  Cari,  3,  20,  92 

Klein,  Melanie,  13 

Kluckhohn,  C,  164,  165,  191,  201,  203,  206 

Kobler,  Arthur,  277,  289,  356 

Kris,  E.  154,  191 

LaForge,  Joan,  257  ff.,  478;  see  also  Har- 
vey, Joan 
LaForge,  Rolfe,  74,  87,  138,  240,  455,  463 
Landis,  C,  314 
Langer,  Suzanne,  156,  191 
Lawrence  D.  H.,  116,  131,  273,  281 
Lawrence,  Herbert,  376,  458 
Leary,  T.,  131,  240,  281,  291,  436,  454,  479 
Lichtenberg,  G.  C,  72 
Lindner,  Robert,  271,  281 
Lodge,  George  T.,  74,  87 

Malamud,  W,,  296-97,  302,  314 

Masserman,  J.  H.,  24,  32,  238 

Mead,  George  H.,  92,  99-101,  102,  131 

Menaker,  Esther,  283,  291 

Merton,  R.  K.,  74 

Miller,  Arthur,  104 

Miller,  Milton  L.,  87 

Moreno,  J,  L.,  102 

Morris,  C.  W.,  35-36,  37 

Mowrer,  O.  H.,  247 

Mullahy,  P.,  32,  48,  49,  328,  331 


501 


502 

Murray,  Heniy,  39,  44,  164-65,  191,  198, 
201,  203,  206,  479 

Naboisek,  H^  87,  299,  340 
Nash,  E.,  281,  331,  337-38,  340 
Newman,  R.,  492 

Parsons,  Talcott,  73,  74,  87 

Plato,  72 

Powdemiaker,  Florence,  278-79,  281 

Powelson,  D.  H.,  14,  93,  131,  163,  191 

Proust,  Marcel,  19,  32 

Raimondi,  P.,  374 

Redlich  F.,  492 

Reich,  Wilhelm,  92 

Reichenbach,  H.  R.,  60-61,  87 

Ribble,  Margaretha,  13 

Rosenthal,  D.,  281,  331,  337-38,  340 

Russell,  Bertrand,  35 

Sapir,  Edward,  101-2,  131 
Sarvis,  Maiy,  148,  153 


INDEX  OF  NAMES 

Siul,  L.  J.,  391,  402 

Shafer,  Roy,  310,  314,  338,  340 

Shakespeare,  William,  18 

Sophocles,  21,  23,  117 

Spitz,  R.  A.,  13 

Stagner,  Ross,  73,  87 

Suczek,  Robert,  138,  375,  455,  458,  463 

SulUvan,  Harry  Stack,  4,  7,  8-10,  12,  13, 

16,  23,  26,  30,  32,  102,  119,  231,  270, 

272,  281 
Sweet,  Blanche,  97,  131 
Symonds,  P.,  202,  206 

Thelen,  H.  A.,  102,  131 
Thomas,  W.  I.,  102 
Thurber,  James,  173-75,  191 
Tomkins,  S.  S.,  191 

Weinshel,  E.,  376 
Wheelwright,  Joseph,  22 
Whitehead,  A.  N.,  120,  131 
Wittgenstein,  L.,  35 
Wolton,  R.  v.,  355 
Wundt,  W.,  102 


Index  of  Subjects 


Page  numbers  in  italics  indicate  illustrations. 


Abnormality,  see  Adjustment-maladjust- 
ment 
Acne;  see  Dermatitis,  overtly  neurotic 
Acne  rosacea;  see  Dermatitis,  unanxious 
Adaptive,  see  Adjustment-maladjustment 
Adjustment-maladjustment,  26,  30-31,  <J5, 
755-36,  197,  247-48 

adaptive-maladaptive  behavior,  6S,  66- 
67,  130,  220;  see  also  Psychotic 
AP  "11"  managerial,   323-24 
AP  "11"  autocratic,  324-26 
BC  "22"  competitive,  333-34 
BC  "22"  narcissistic,  334 
DE  "33"  aggressive,  341^2,  344 
DE  "33"  sadistic,  341-42,  344,  345 
FG  "44"  rebelhous,  270 
FG  "44"  distrustful,  276-78 
HI  "55"  self-effacing,  282 
HI  "55"  masochistic,  282 
JK  "66"  docile,  292 
JK  "66"  dependent,  292 
LM  "77"  cooperative,  303-4 
LM  "77"  overconventional,  304-6 
NO  "88"  responsible,  315-16 
NO  "88"  hypemormal,  315-17 

and  diagnosis,  219-20,  228,  231-33,  235, 
386-88,  492 

continuum  in  relativity  of,  20-23,  26,  38 

factors  in  personality  theory,  17-32 

measurement  of,  217-28 

pathology  error,  17,  25 

principle  of,  26 

psychopathic,  347-48 

qualitative,   28-29 

quantitative,  27 

theories  of 

eff'ect  of  cultural  values  on,  26-27 
Freudian,  19-20,  22 
Fromm's,  22-23,  24,  30 
Homey's,  22,  30 
Jung's,  20 
religious,  18 
social,  27,  29 
Sullivan's,  23,  30 


Affiliation 

vocabulary  for,  29 
Aggressive  behavior;  see  Sadistic  "33"  DE 

behavior 
Alcoholism,  326,  361 

Alopecia   areata;   see  Dermatitis,  unanx- 
ious 
Anxiety,  17,  22,  23,  93,  94,  183,  188,  203, 
235,  271,  303,  307-8,  326,  336,  344;  see 
also  Security  operations 
and  postponement  of  impulse,  165 
and  psychotics,  360 
and  symbols,  162-64 
basis  of,   15 
hysteria,  297 

in   interpersonal  reflexes,  451 
motivates  behavior,  19 
neurosis,  296-97 
-panic,  180,  356;  see  also  Samples,  clinic 

psychotic 
Sullivan's  theory  of,  8,  119 
survival,  14,  120-22,  202-3,  209 
AP;   see   Autocratic   "11"  AP  behavior; 

Variables,  interpersonal 
Atopic    dermatitis;   see   Dermatitis,   self- 
inflicted 
Autocratic    "11"   AP   behavior,   6S,    117, 
120,  130,  755,  139,  142,  149,  198,  218- 
20,  233,  235-38,  323-31 
adaptive,  323-24 
and  compulsivity,  327-29 
and  narcissistic      behavior      compared, 

333,  337 
and  obsession,  328-29 
and  psychosomatic  research,  380  ff. 
and  psychotics,  368,  372 
and  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis,  327- 

29 
and  therapy  group,  428-2P,  430,  432 
and  top  management,  407,  420 
at  Level  I,  104,  372,  380 
at  Level  II,  137,  382 
at  Level  III,  170  ff.,  384,  481  ff. 
check-list  items,  456 


503 


504 

Autocratic  "11"  AP  hehzvior-C ontinued 
clinical  manifestations,  325-27 
in  Death  of  a  Salemtan,  104 
in  Freud's  "Irma"  dream,  172 
in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty," 

173-74 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  350-51 
maladaptive,  324-26,  345 
purpose  of,  325 
Automaton  conformity,  neurotic  mech- 
anism, 8 
Avenger,  the,  371-72 

BC;  see  Narcissistic  "22"  BC  behavior; 

Variables,   interpersonal 
Blacky  projective  test;  see  Level  III-B 
Buffoon,  284 

Catatonia,  55,  127,  236-37 
Character 
and  symptom,  24,  25,  26 
moral,  29-30 
theories  of 
Freud's,  8 
Fromm's,  8 
Horney's,  8 
Child   guidance,    145-48 
Class  statements,  241 
Codes 

and  interpersonal  reflexes,  445-52 
diagnostic,   211,  225-26,   257  ff.,   265  ff., 
473,  493-500 
numerical,  227-28 
of  interpersonal  group  dynamics,  410 
of  interpersonal  mechanisms,   103-5 
of  Levels  of  interpersonal  behavior,  78- 

81 
of  MMPI  scales,  439 
of  typical  TAT  themes,  170-71,  466-70 
of  variability  indices,  252,  254-56 

key  to,  256 
psychosomatic  samples,   374 
top-management  executives,  405 
Commentator,  the,  369-70 
Communication 
conscious;  see  Level  II 
interpersonal,  99-103 
linguistic;  see  Language 
public;  see  Level  I 
Compass  of  motives,  73 
Compulsive  behavior,  327-28 
Conflict  axis,  181 

Conflict,    interlevel;   see   Variability,    in- 
dices 
Conscious    communication,     description; 
see  Level  II 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

Conscious   disidentification,   489 
maternal,  140,  254 
paternal,  254 
spouse,  254 
therapist,  142-43,  254 
Conscious  fusion,  252 
Conscious  idealization,  252,  431 
Conscious  identification,    140,    251,    252, 
284,  431,  434,  489 
maternal,  254 
paternal,  140,  254 
spouse,  254 
therapist,  254 
Conscious-"Preconscious"  fusion;  see  Dif- 
fusion; Fusion 
Consciousness,  sublevels  of,  148-51 
Consensual  validation,  9,  35-38,  48,  61 
Control    sample;    see    Samples,    normal 

control,  obese  middle-class  females 
Cooperative   behavior;   see   Overconven- 

tional  "77"  LM  behavior 
Counterphobic  behavior,  181,  337 

compensatory,  179 
Countertransference,   143,   144 
Cross-level  diffusion,  255 
Cross-level      disidentification,     cross-sex, 
maternal,     other,     paternal,     spouse, 
therapist,  254 
Cross-level  identification,  252,  431 
cross-sex,     maternal,     other,     paternal, 
spouse,  therapist,  254 
Cultural  samples,  129,  130,  152,  190,  280- 
81,   290-91,   299-300,    312-13,    321-22, 
330-31,  33&-40,  349-50 
Cynic  and  Tough  Guy,  the,  367-h59 

DE;  see  Sadistic  "33"  DE  behavior;  Vari- 
ables, interpersonal 
Defense  mechanisms,  247-48,  252 
Dependent  "66"  JK  behavior,  6S,  68,  1 17, 
J3S,   139,   143,   145,  204,  219-20,  225- 
26,  231,  233,  235-38,  292-302 
and  anxiety  hysteria,  297-98 
and  anxiety  neurosis,  296-97 
and  hypochondriasis,  297-98 
and  neurasthenia,  297 
and  phobia,  294-98 
and  psychosomatic  research,  382  ff. 
and  psychotics,  362,  364,  366-67,  368 
and  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis,  296- 

98 
and  therapy  group,  433 
and  top  management,  407,  413,  424 
at  Level    I,   92-93,    104,    362,    364,    366, 

481  ff. 
at  Level  II,  137,  382 
at  Level  III,  170  ff. 
check-list  items,  457 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 


505 


clinical  definition  of,  294-96 

effect  of,  293-94 

in  Death  of  a  Salesman,  104 

in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty," 

174 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  299-300 
purpose  of,  292-93 
research    findings,    298-300 
therapeutic  handling  of,  300-2 
Wanderer,  the,  361-63 
Dermatitis 

and    psychosomatic    research,    374-77, 

379  ff. 
industrial,   377 
neuro-,  290 
overtly  neurotic,   376-77 

acne,  376 

at  Level  I,  379-81 

at  Level  II,  381-84 

at  Level  III,  384-86 

clinical  implications,  393-94 

multilevel  pattern,  393 

psoriasis,  376 

seborrheic,  376 
samples,  129,  130,  152,  190,  280-81,  290- 

91,  299-300,  312-13,  330-31,  338,  349- 

50,   374,   376-77,   379 ff. 
self-inflicted,  377 

at  Level  I,  379-81 

at  Level  II,  381-84 

at  Level  III,  384-86 

atopic,    376-77 

clinical  implications,  395-96 

eczematous,  376-77 

multilevel  pattern,  394-95 

neurotic  excoriations,  376-77 

otitis  externa,  376-77 

pruritis,   376-77 
unanxious,  377 

acne  rosacea,  376-77 

alopecia  areata,  376-77 

at  Level  I,  379-81 

at  Level  II,  381-84 

at  Level  111,  384-86 

clinical  implications,  396-97 

herpes  simplex,  376-77 

hyperhydrotic  eczema,   376-77 

lupus  erythematosus,  376-77 

multilevel  pattern,  396 

urticaria,    376-77 

warts,  376-77 
Destructiveness,   neurotic   mechanism,    8, 

72 
Devaluation;    see   also    Conscious   ideali- 
zation;   "Preconscious"    devaluation; 

"Preconscious"  idealization 


maternal,  255 
paternal,  255 
spouse,  255 
therapist,  255 
Diagnosis,  439-500,  see  also  Levels 
and  adjustment,    219-20,    228,    231-33, 

235,  492 
and  psychiatric  nosology,  207-8,  229-38, 

274-76,   286-87,  294-98,   306-10,   317- 

20,  325-27,  327-29,  335-38,  345,  347^8 
and  therapeutically    relevant   variables, 

208 
differential,  208 
double-level,  266-67,  473 
family,   139-42,   145-50 
four-level,  228,  267-68 
functional,  53-56,  214  ff. 

aim  of,  216 
in  TAT  validation  study,  473  ff. 
interpersonal,  207-38 

and  psychiatric,  230-37,  274-76,  286- 
87,  294-98,  306-10,  317-19,  325-27, 
335-36,  345 

norms  for,  493-500 

of  psychotics,  354-72 

use  of,  210  ff. 
multilevel,  107,  225-28,  265-68,  360,  368- 

400,  429-36,  480-89 
of  clinic  admission  patients,  397-98 
of  dermatitis  patients,  393-96 
of  hypertensives,  390 
of  interpersonal  types,  265-350,  481 
of  neurotic  patients,  399 
of  normality,  219-20,  386-88 
of  obese  women,  391-92 
of  others,   139-40 
of  presenting  operations,  217-21 
of  symptoms,  235-37;  see  also  Symptom 
of  ulcer  patients,  388 
of  underlying  operations,  222-24 
"other,"  139-42;  see  also  Relationships, 

reciprocal 
purpose  of,  207-9 
record  booklet  for  interpersonal,  482- 

88 
self,  138-39,  152 
single-level,  106,  266 
therapist,   145 
triple-level,  267 
verbal,  226-27 
Diagnostic  code,  211,  225-26,  257  ff.,  265 

ff.,  473,  493-500 
numerical,  227-28 
Diagnostic  continuum,  237-38 
Diagnostic   language,   56-58,   229-38,    304 
Diagnostic  profile,  211-12,  214-15 
Diagnostic  report,  212-15,  362,  364,  366, 

368,  370,  371-72,  480-92 


5o6 

Diagnostic  repon-Continued 
and  motivation  for  psychotherapy,  480, 

489,  492 
and  "preconscious"  conflicts,  480,  489 
and  prognosis  of  response  to  psycho- 
therapy, 480,  490 
Diagnostic  types,  listed,  220 
Diffusion;  see  also  Cross-level  diffusion; 
Displacement;  Fusion 
cross-sex,  255 
maternal,  254 
paternal,  255 
Disequation 
maternal-paternal,   254 
maternal-spouse,  254 
maternal-therapist,  254 
paternal-spouse,  254 
paternal-therapist,  254 
spouse-therapist,  254 
Disidentification;  see  Conscious  disiden- 
tification;     Cross-level     disidentifica- 
tion; "Preconscious"  disidentification 
Displacement,  252,  253,  255;  see  also  Dif- 
fusion 
Distrustful   "44"   FG   behavior,    65,    110, 
116-17,  130,  13S,  139,  150,  194,  219-20, 
225-26,  231,  233,  235-38,  268-81,  289 
and  psychosomatic  research,  384  ff. 
and  psychotics,   360  ff. 
and  schizoid,  235,  238 
maladjustment,  276-77 
psychosis,    277-78 
and  therapy  group,  432 
and  top  management,  407,  412,  418,  421 
at  Level  I,  93,  95-96,  104-5 
at  Level  III,  170  ff.,  384 
check-list  items,  456 
clinical  manifestation  of,  274-76 
effect  of,  272-73 
in  Death  of  a  Salesman,  104 
in  Freud's    "Irma"    dream,    172 
in  nursery  school  situation,  105 
in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty," 

173-74 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  175-76,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  280-81 
purpose  of,  269 
research  findings,  279-81 
Distrustful  personality,  270,  272 
Docile  behavior;  see  Dependent  "66"  JK 

behavior 
Doctor-patient  relationship,  93-94,  354 
Doctor's  assistant,  329,  337 
Dominance-submission,  107,  430,  489,  493- 
500 
formula,  68h59 
predictive  indices,  440-43,  445,  473-74 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 
Dreams;  see  Level  III-D 

Eczema,    hyperhydrotic;   see   Dermatitis, 

unanxious 
Eczematous    dermatitis;    see    Dermatitis, 

self-inflicted 
Ego 
factors,  216 

ideal,   80-81,   84,   200-6,    303,    362,   481, 
484~8S,  489,  491 
coding  of,  256 
psychology,  193 
Equation,  252,  431 
maternal-paternal,  254 
maternal-spouse,  254 
matemal-therapist,  254 
paternal-spouse,   254 
paternal-therapist,  254 
spouse-therapist,  254 
Exploitive  character,  8,  337 

Familial   equation,   253,  481,   489-92;   see 

also  Disequation;  Equation 
FamUy  dynamics,  481,  485,  488-92 
Fantasy;  see  also  Level  III-F 

and  overconventional  behavior,  311 

classification  of  materials,  167 

classification    of    person,    169 
FG;   see   Distrustful   "44"   FG  behavior; 

Variables,    interpersonal 
Flat  affect,  274 
Formulae 

diagnostic,  228 

MMPI  predictive,  107,  440-41 

summarizing  interpersonal  behavior,  68- 
69 
Freudian,  7,  42,  71,  158,  186,  252 

defense  mechanisms,  86 

bbido  theory,  12 

psychosexual  theory,  9-11 

theory  of  normality,  19 
Frigidity,  308,  319 

Fusion;  see  also  Diffusion;  "Preconscious" 
fusion 

conscious-"preconscious,"   252 

cross-sex,  255 

maternal,  254 

paternal,  255 

General  Manager,  the,  405,  406,  418-20 
seen  by   Personnel   Manager,  419-20 
seen  by  Production  Manager,  419-20 
seen  by    psychologist,   419-20 
seen  by  Sales  Manager,  410,  413, 

419-20 
sees  Personnel  Manager,  418-20 
sees  Production  Manager,  418-19 
sees  Sales  Manager,  418-19 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 


507 


Group  behavior 

distrustful,  275-76,  277-78 

psychotic,  361  S. 
Group  personality,  353,  426-27 
Group  top  management;  see  Management 

group 
Group  dynamics 

analysis  of,  408-9,  432-36 

and  psychotherapy  group,  426-36 

and  top  management,  403-25 

indices,  414 

record  booklet,  411-17 

verbal  summaries,  415 
Group  resistance,  353 

and  multilevel  personality  indices,  431- 
36 

balancing  of,  427-28 

measurement  of,  429-30 

prediction  of,  426-27,  428-30 
Group  therapy 

at  Level  I-R,  445-53 

at  Level  I-S,  453-54 

prediction  of  behavior;  see  Level  I-P 

samples,  129,  130,  152,  190,  280-81,  291, 
299-300,  312-13,  321-22,  330-31,  338, 
340,  349-50,  426-36 

selection  of  patients,  427-28 

Hate;  see  Love-hate 

Hebephrenic,  55 

"Help-rejecting  complainer,"   275-76, 

337 
Hero 
generic,  323 

Level  III,  80,  82,  167-68,  177-80,  193-95, 
222-28,  254,  357,  431-52,  465,  466  ff., 
481 
norms,  497 

predicts    change    in    overt    behavior, 
472  S. 
Herpes  simplex;  see  Dermatitis,  unanxious 
HI;  see   Masochistic   "55"  HI  behavior; 

Variables,  interpersonal 
Hoarding  character  type,  8 
Hoess,  Colonel,  184 
Hostility,  199,  275 
and  psychotics,  357  ff. 
and  sadistic  behavior,  341-50 
vocabulary  for,  29 
Hypernormal  "88"  NO  behavior,  65,  117, 
130,  135,  139,  149-50,  219-20,  231-33, 
235-38,  315-22 
adaptive,  315 

and  psychosomatic  research,   380  ff. 
and  psychotics,  356  ff. 
and  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis,  319- 

20 
and  therapy  group,  428-2P,  430 


and  top    management,    406,    401,    412, 

413,  420,  423 
at  Level  I,  105,  380 
at  Level  II,  382 
at  Level  III,  171  ff. 
check-list  items,  457 
clinical  manifestations  of,  317-19 
effect  of,  317 

in  typical  TAT  themes,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  321-22 
maladaptive,  315-16 
research  findings,  320-22 
Hypertension 
and  psychosomatic  research,  374  ff. 

at  Level  I,  41,  379-81 

at  Level  II,  381-84 

at  Level  III,  384-86 

clinical  implication,  391 

multilevel  pattern,  390-91 
samples,  129,  152,  190,  280-81,  291,  299- 

300,  312-13,  321-22,  330-31,  338,  340, 

349-50,  374,  375 
Hypochondriasis,  295,  297-98 
Hysterical  personality,  185,  189,  235,  238 
Hysterics,  108,  207,  209,  229,  233,  235,  295, 

300-1,  308,  310-14 

Ideal  ego,  80,  81,  84,  200-<5,  303,  362,  481, 
484-8S,  489,  491 
coding  of,  256 
Ideal  self,  201 

Idealization;   see   Conscious   idealization; 
Devaluation;  "Preconscious"  idealiza- 
tion 
Identification,  145 
conscious;  see  Conscious  identification 
cross-level;    see    Cross-level    identifica- 
tion 
maternal,  255 
paternal,  255 
"preconscious";      see      "Preconscious" 

identification 
spouse,  255 
tnerapist,  255 
Iflund  projective  test,  198-99 
Impotence,  319-27 
Inadequate  personality,  207 
Indices 
of  discrepancy,  493,  498-99 
qualitative,  473  flF.;  see  also  Diagnostic 

code 
quantitative,  473  ff.;  see  also   Varia- 
bility indices,  measurement  of 
predictive,  440-52,  473  ff. 
sociometric,  453-54 
Instinct,  12,  23-24,  72 
Erikson's  theory  of,  10-11 


5o8 

Instinct— Cow  tinuei 

Freudian  theory  of  personality,  7-9,  19- 
20 
Intensity;  see  Measurement,  of  intensity 
Interpersonal  behavior,  4,  90-238,  240;  see 
also  Levels;  Measurement 

and  reciprocal  relationship;  see  Rela- 
tionships, reciprocal 

classification    (16  mechanisms),   64-66, 
219-20 

diagnosis   of;   see   Diagnosis,   interper- 
sonal 

functional  diagnosis  of,  53-56,  58 

importance  of,  12-13 

logic  of  interaction,  39 

prediction  of;  see  Prediction 

principle  of,  15-16 

purpose  of,  15 

stability  vs.  flexibihty,   121-22 

Sullivan's  theories  of,  8-9 

summarization  of,  67,  228 
circular  grid,  69,  493 
formulae,  68-69 

variability  of,  75 

vocabulary  of,  29 
Interpersonal  check  list,  29,  30,  138,  197, 
205-6,  455-63,  492;  see  also  Level  I-S; 
Level  II-C;  Level  V-C 

and  group  dynamics,  405,  430,  453 

and    TAT    validation    study,    473  ff. 

derivation  of,  457-58 

development  and  revision  of,  458-61 

formulae,  69 

intensity  of  items,  455,  458 

internal  consistency  of,  461-63 

intervariable  correlation,  462 

items   listed,  456-57 

norms,  463,  495 

samples,  458  ff. 
Interpersonal  communication,  history  of, 

99-103 
Interpersonal  dimension,  compared  with 

variability  dimension,  244-46 
Interpersonal    Fantasy    Test;    see    Level 

III-IFT 
Interpersonal   mechanism;    see   Interper- 
sonal reflex 
Interpersonal  perception,  252 

ana  interpersonal  reflex,  450 
Interpersonal  reflex,  91,  96,  110,  123,  130 

and  physiological  reflexes,  97-99 

and  self-determination,  115-18 

cause  of,  118-22 

doctor-patient  relationship,  93-SH 

listing  of,  65,  103 

measurement,  105  flF.,  445-53 

need  not  be  conscious,  98-99 

routine  patterns,  109 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

scoring,  104-5,  137-38 

teaching  reflex,  94 
Interpersonal  role,  109-10 
Interpersonal  theorists,  5,  6,  7,  102-3 

Bales,  102-3 

Bion,  102-3 

Dollard,  102 

Erikson,  10-12 

Fromm,  7 

Homey,  7 

Mead,  99-101 

Moreno,  102 

Sapir,  101-2 

Sullivan,  8-10,  102,  119 

Thelen,  102 

Thomas,  102 
Interpersonal  typology,  220,  265  ff .;  see 
also  Autocratic  "11"  AP  behavior; 
Dependent  "66"  JK  behavior;  Diag- 
nosis, multilevel;  Distrustful  "44"  FG 
behavior;  Hypemormal  "88"  NO 
behavior;  Masochistic  "55"  HI  be- 
havior; Narcissistic  "22"  EC  be- 
havior; Overconventional  "77"  LM 
behavior;  Sadistic  "33"  DE  behavior 

organization  of,  268 

presenting  operations,  217-21,  237 

underlying  operations,  217,  222-28, 
237 
Interpersonal    variables;    see   Variability; 

Variables 
Intrapersonal    variables;    see    Variability; 

Variables 
Introjection,  201 
IQ  tests,  5 

JK;   see   Dependent   "66"   JK   behavior; 

Variables,   interpersonal 
Jungian,  23 
theories,  20-22,  156,  158 

Kraepelinian  diagnosis,  229-38 
Kraepelinian  psychiatry,  10,  159 
Kraepelinian  terms,  209,  236-37,  337 

Language 
of  behavior,  99-102,  207-8 
and  variability,  34-35,  38-39,  241  ff. 
attributive  nature  of,  133-34 
class  statements,  139,  241-46 
diagnostic,  56-58,  229-38,  304 
functional,  56 
operational  definitions  of  terms,  47- 

48,  76-77,  81-82,  234 
protocol,  34-35 
reflexive,  100-1 

relationship  statements,   139,  241-46 
selection  of  variables.  38-39 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

selection  of  words  for  Interpersonal 
Check  List,  29-30,  457  ff. 

significant  symbol,  100-1 

symbols  as,  161-62 
scientific,  35-38 

empirical   propositions,    35-37,   245 

formal  propositions,  35-37,  245 

pragmatics,  36 

semantic  rules,  36 

semantics,  36 

syntactics,  36 
Level  I   (Public  Communication),  76-80, 

81,    83-84,   91-131,    211-12,    242,    244, 

247,  254,  266,  272,  279,  284,  287,  290, 

298,   308,   310-11,   319,   320,   348,  439- 

54,  455,  481,  484 
and  Level  II,  95,  98-99,  115,   117,   134, 

136,   144-45,   149,  251,  254,  256,  266- 

68,  348,  431-33 
and  Level  IH,  160-61,  163,  168,  169-71, 

177,  183-84,  188,  223-28 
and  Level  IV,  194-98 
and  Level  V,  203 
and  psycho  tics,  356  ff. 
avoidance  of  themes,  196-99 
diagnosis  of,  217-21,  441-44 
Level  I-M,  78,  81,  105-8,  205,  218,  220-21, 

224,     234,     237,     269,     289-90,     311, 

491 
and  psychosomatic  research 

all  samples,  379-81 

clinic  admissions,  398 

dermatitis 
overtly  neurotic,  394 
self-inflicted,  394-95 
unanxious,  397 

hypertensive,  390 

neurotic,  399-400 

normal  controls,  386-87 

norms,  494 

obesity,  392 

psychotic,  400-1 

ulcer,  388-89 
and  psychotics,  355  ff. 
estimates     of    symptomatic     behavior, 

106-7,  439-44,  453 
formulae,  107,  440-41 
mcidence   in  cultural  samples,   129-30, 

279^0,  290-91,  299,  312-13,  321,  330, 

338-39,  349 
Level  I-P,  78,  106,  426-30 

prediction  indices,  108-9,  444-45 
Level  I-R,  78,  81,  82,  95-96,  105-7,  445-53, 

454 
Level  I-S,  78,  81,  106-8,  179,  194,  287 
and  Level  I-M,  443-44 
and  Level  I-P,  445 
and  management  group,  405  ff. 


509 

and  psychotherapy  group,  426,  429-30, 

432 
and  psychotic  samples,  356 
indices,  453-54 
Level  I-T,  78,  81,  106 
Level  II       (Conscious      Communication, 
Description),  76,  78,  81,  83-85,  132- 
53,  171-73,  205,  211-12,  242,  244,  248, 
253-54,  256,  266-68,  272,  279,  287,  289- 

90,  298,  308,  310,  348,  455-63,  481, 
484-85 

and  Level  \,  95,  98-99,    115,   117,   134, 

136,   144-45,   149,   251,   254,  256,   348, 

431-33 
and  Level  III,  151,   161,  164,  168,  170- 

71,   173,   177,   183-90,  223-28,  246-47, 

254-56,  473  ff. 
and  Level  IV,  194-98 
and  Level  V,  202,  255 
and   psychotics,   357  ff. 
avoidance  of  themes,  196-99 
diagnosis  of  217-21 
Level  II-A,  78,  81,  136-37 
Level  II-C,  78,  81,  82,   136-39,   148,   151, 

176-77,  182,  194,  204,  218,  220-21,  224, 

234,  237,  269,  334,  481,  485,  491 
and  management  group,  405,  406,  410 
and  psychosomatic  research 

all  samples,  381-84 

chnic  admissions,  398 

dermatitis 
overtly  neurotic,   394 
self-inflicted,  394-95 
unanxious,  391 

hypertensive,  390 

neurotic,  399-400 

normal  controls,  386-87 

obesity,  392 

psychotic,  400-1 

ulcer,  388-89 
and  psychotics,  357  ff. 
and  therapy  group,  432 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,    151-52, 

280-81,  291,  300,  313,  321-22,  331,  339- 

40,  350 
norms,  495 

scoring  of  interpersonal  traits,  137-40 
Level  II-Di,  78,  81,  136 
Level  II-Ti,  78,  81,  136-37,  151 
Level  III     (Private    Symbolization,    Per- 
ception), 76-77,  79,  81-82,  83-85,  154- 

91,  212,  244,  253,  431,  464-79,  481, 
484-85;  see  also  Thematic  Appercep- 
tion Test 

and  Level  I,  95,  114,  160-61,  163,   168, 

169^71,  177,  183-84,  188 
and  Level  II,  151,  164,  168,  170-71,  173, 

177,   183-90,  246-47,  254-56,  473  ff. 


5IO 

Level  III— Continued 
and  Level  IV,  186,  194-98 
and  Level  V,  200,  256 
and  psychotics,  357  flF. 
avoidance  of  themes,  196-99 
classification  of  fantasy  person,  169 
clinical  use  of,  189-91 
depth  of  symbol  instrument,  184-88 
diagnosis  of,  222-28 
Hero,   80,  82,    167-68,    177-80,    193-95, 
222-28,  254,  357,  431-32,  465,  466  ff., 
481 
norms,  496 

predicts   change   in   overt  behavior, 
472  ff. 
meaning  of,  177 

Other,   80,  82,    167-68,  177-80,   193-95, 
222-28,  254,  431-32,  465,  466-70 
norms,  497 
relationship  principle,  176 
symbols,  155-57,  169-70 
Level  III-B,  79,  82,    166 
Level  III-D,  79,  82,   166 

Freud's  "Irma"  dream,  171-73 
Level  III-F,  79,  82,  166,  224 
in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty," 
173-75 
Level  m-i,  79,  82,  166 
Level  III-IFT,  79,  82,  167 
Level  III-M,  79,  82,   166 
Level  III-T,  79,  82,  167,  182-84,  223,  224, 
464-79,  485,  491;  see  also  Level  III, 
Hero  and  Other;  Thematic  Apper- 
ception Test 
and  psychosomatic  research 
all  samples,  384-86 
clinic  admissions,  398 
dermatitis 
overdy  neurotic,  394 
self-inflicted,  394-95 
unanxious,  391 
hypertensive,   390 
neurotic,  399-400 
normal  controls,  386-87 
obesity,  392 
psychotic,  400-2 
ulcer,  388-89 
and  psychotics,  357  ff. 
and  therapy  group,  432-34 
guide  for  rating,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  190-91 
norms,  496-97 

prediction  of  change,  472  ff. 
scoring,  175-79 
validation  study,  472  ff. 
Level    IV    (Unexpressed    Unconscious), 
76,  80,  82,  83-84,  192-99,  212,  224,  252 
and  Level  I,  194-98  . 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

and  Level  II,  194-98 

and  Level  III,  186,  195-98 

criteria  for  defining,  192 
Level  V    (Level  of  Values),  76,  80-81, 
82,  84-85,  200-6,  212,  224 

and  Level  I,  203 

and  Level  II,  202,  255 

and  Level  III,  200,  256 

functional  value  of,  205 

ideal  self,  201 

interpersonal  ideals,  203-4 

limitations  of  score,  205-6 
Level  V-C,  81,  83,  204 
Level  V-Di,  81,  82,  204 
Level  V-Ti,  81,  82,  204 
Level  of  values;  see  Level  V 
Levels;  see  also  Diagnosis,  multilevel 

described,  75  ff. 

logic  of,  42-43 
LM;  see  Overconventional  "77"  LM  be- 
havior; Variables,  interpersonal 
Locus  of  responsibility,  18-24 
Love-hate,  107,  430,  489,  493 

formula,  68-69 

predictive  indices,  440-43,  445,  473-74 
Lupus  erythematosus;  see  Dermatitis,  un- 
anxious 

Maladaptive;    see    Adjustment-maladjust- 
ment, adaprive-maladaptive  behavior 
Maladjustment;    see    Adjustment-malad- 
justment 
Management  group,  403-25 

analysis  of  dynamics,  408-9 

Level  I-S,  405-8 

Level  II-C  perceptions  of  self,  405,  406, 
408 

network  of  relationships,  409-25 
Manic  behavior,   324 
Manic-depressive,  236-37 
Marketing  character  type,  8 
Masochism 

and  obsessive   neurosis,   287 

and  sadism,  130,  288-89 

neurotic  mechanism,  8,  433-34 
Masochistic    "55"    HI   behavior,    65,   91, 
117,  119,  130,  135,  139,  143,  197,  219- 
20,  225-26,  231-33,  235-39,  282 

and  psychosomatic  research  380  ff. 

and  psychotics,  365,  366-67 

and  therapy    group,    428-2P,   430,    432, 
433 

and  top  management,  413,  421,  424 

at  Level  I,  95-96,  104-5,  380 

at  Level  II,  137 

at  Level  III,  170  ff. 

check-list  items,  456 

clinical  definition  of,  286-87 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 


51 


in  Death  of  a  Salesman,  104 

in  Freud's  "Irma"  dream,   172 

in  nursery  school  situation,  105 

in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Alitty," 

173-74 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  175-76,  466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  290-91 
purpose  of,  282 
research  findings,  290-91 
Measurement,  39,  45,  67  ff.,  240,  439-500, 
see  also  Diagnosis,  Diagnostic  code, 

etc.;  Variability 
and  clinicians,    114-15 
and  psychologists,  112-13 
and  technicians,    114-15 
and  variables,   39 
conflict  axis,  178 
depth  of  instrument,  186-88 
Level   I,   77-78,   81,    103-6,    107,    108-9, 

217-28,  439-54 
Level  I-M,  439-44,  494 
Level  I-P,  444-45 
Level  1-R,  444-53 
Level  I-S,  453-54 
Level  n,  78,  81,    136-38,    151-52, 

217-28 
Level  III,  79,  81-82,  166-91,  222-28,  464- 

79 
Level  III-C,  495 
Level  III-D,  171-73 
Level  III-F,  173-75 
Level  III-T,  175-76,  496-97 
Level  IV,  80,  82,  193-99 

omission  scores,  195 
Level  V,  80,  82,  203^ 
molar  scoring,  176 
molecular  scoring,  175 
multilevel,   41-44,   81  ff,    241  ff. 
of  adjustment,  217-28 
of  check-list  reliability,  460-63 
of  conscious  identification,  140-42 
of  discrepancy  between  the  diagnostic 

codes,  498-99 
of  discrepancy  vi^ith   the  interpersonal 

system,   257-60 
of  dominance-submission,    68-69,    440- 

43 
of  group  behavior,  444-45 
of  group   dynainics,  403-36 
of  group   resistance,  429-30 
of  ideal-self  discrepancy,  205 
of  intensity,    66,     104-5,    224-25,    228, 

260-61,   455,   458,   498-500 
of  interpersonal  attributes,  134  ff. 
of  interpersonal  reflexes 

at  Level  I,  105  ff. 

scoring,  104-5,  137-38,  445-53 


of  love-hate,  68-69,  440-43 

of  "Other,"  39 

of  self-other,  83-84,  136-^2,  144-45 

of  significant    avoidance    of    interper- 
sonal themes,  195-99 

of  symbols,   167 

of  therapist's  misperceptions,   143-45 

of  transference,  142^3,  149-50 

of  unexpressed  themes,  193-95 

of  variability  indices,  85-86,  257-61 

on  a  continuum,  20-21,  23,  26,  38,  241 

patient  as  instrument  of,  112-13 

power-passivity  axis,   178 

psychologist  as  instrument  of,  112-15 
Minnesota  Multiphasic  Personality  In- 
ventory, 234,  276,  279,  286-87,  290, 
294,  298,  306,  311-12,  329,  336,  339, 
347,  348,  359-60,  439^5,  481,  492;  see 
also  Level  I-M;  Level  I-P;  Level 
III-M 

norms,  494 

predictive  formulae,  107,  440-41 

scales,  439 

standardization  sample,  441 
Misperception,  248-49,  489;  see  also  Self- 
deception 

and  management  group,  406  ff . 

and  therapy  group,  431,  435 
Multilevel    interpersonal    diagnosis,    107, 
225-28,  265-68,  360,  429-36,  480-89 

Naboisek  study,  299,  340 

Narcissistic  "22"  BC  behavior,  55,  68,  111, 

117,  119-20,  135,  139,  149-50,  219-20, 

225-26,  233,  235-38,  322,  332-40,  433- 

35 
and  autocratic  behavior,  compared,  333, 

337 
and  psychosomatic  research,  382  ff . 
and  psychotics,  370,  372 
and     standard     psychiatric     definition, 

337-38 
and   therapy  group,  428-25),   430,   432, 

433 
and    top    management,    401,   413,    418, 

420,  422,  423 
at  Level  I,  93,  95-96,  103-5,  370 
at  Level  II,  137,  372,  382 
at  Level  III,  170  ff.,  384 
check-list  items,  456 
clinical  manifestation  of,  335-36 
effect  of,  334 

in  Death  of  a  SalesTnan,  104 
in  Freud's  "Irma"  dream,  172 
in  nursery  school  situation,  105 
in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty," 

173-74 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  175,  466-70 


512 

Narcissistic  "22"  BC  hehzvior-C ontinued 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  338-40 
purpose  of,  332-34 
research  findings,  338-40 
Neurasthenia,  297 
Neurasthenic,  180,  233 
Neurosis,   25;   see   also   Ad)ustment-mal- 
adjustment 
and  autocratic  behavior,  325-26 
organ,  298,  318 
Neurotic  excoriations;  see  Dermatitis, 

self-inflicted 
Neurotic  personality 
Homey's  theory  of,  7 
interpersonal  reflexes,  444-53 
MMPI  profile,  442^3 
rated  by  therapy  group,  453-54 
Neurotic  sample,  374,  377-78 
Neurotics,  and  psychosomatics,  373  ff. 
at  Level  I,  379-81 
at  Level  II,  381-84 
at  Level  III,  384-86 
clinical  implications,  400 
multilevel  pattern,  399-400 
Newtonian  physics,  48 
Nice  Guy,  the  365-67 
NO;  see  Hypemormal  "88"  NO  behavior; 

Variables,  interpersonal 
Noninterpersonal  behavior,  4,  159 
Noninterpersonal  systems  of  psychology, 

4-6 
Noninterpersonal  variables,  159-60 
Normal  (medical)  controls 
and  psychosomatic  research,  373  ff . 
at  Level  I,  379-81 
at  Level  II,  381-^4 
at  Level  III,  384-86 
clinical  implications,  387-88 
multilevel  pattern,  386-87 
samples,  129,  152,  190,  280-81,  291,  299- 
300,  312-13,  321-22,  330-31,  338,  340, 
349-50,  374,  377 
Normality;     see     Adjustment-maladjust- 
ment 
Norms 
for  interpersonal  diagnosis,  493-500 
for  variability  diagnosis,  493-500 
Interpersonal  Check  List  (Level  II-C), 

463,  495 
MMPI  (Level  I-M),  494 
TAT  (Level  III-T),  472 
Hero,  496 
Other,  497 
Nuclear  conflicts,  Erikson,  11 

Obesity 
and  psychosomatics,  374  ff. 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

at  Level  I,  379-81 
at  Level  II,  381-84 
at  Level  III,  384-86 
clinical  implications,  392-93 
multilevel  pattern,  391-92 
samples,   129,   152,   190,   280-81,  290-91, 
299-300,   312-13,   321-22,   330-31,   338, 
340,  349-50,  374,  375 
Obsessive,  229,  235,  295,  298,  301 
and  autocratic  behavior,  326 
and  schizoid  behavior,  278-79 
compulsive  phenomenon,  207,  288-89 
neurosis  and  masochism,  287 
personality,  238,  267 
Organ  neurosis,  298,  318 
Other;  see  Level  III,  Other 
Other-misperception,  254 
Other-perception,  254 
Otitis  externa;  see  Dermatitis,  self-inflicted 
Overconventional  "77"  LM  behavior,  55, 
117,  120,  13S,  139,  149-50,  218,  219-20, 
231,  233,  235-38,  303-14 
adaptive  303-4 
and  psychotics,  356  ff. 
and  standard  psychiatric  diagnosis,  310- 

11 
and  top  management,  418,  420,  421,  424 
at  Level  I,  104 
at  Level  II,  137 
at  Level  III,  171  ff. 
check-list  items,  457 
clinical  manifestation  of,  306-10 
effect  of,  305-6 

in  typical  TAT  themes,  467-68 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  312-13 
maladaptive,  304-6 
purpose  of,  305 
research  findings,  311 

Paradigm  of  motivational  process,  73,  74 

Paranoia,  236-37,  319-20 

Paranoid,  236 

Parataxic  experience,  9 

Pathology  error,  17,  23,  25 
clinical  error,  30 
of  Freud,  20 

Personality,  15,  156 

"11";  see  Autocratic  "11"  AP  behavior 
"22";  see  Narcissistic  "22"  BC  behavior 
"33";  see  Sadistic  "33"  DE  behavior 
"44";  see  Distrustful  "44"  FG  behavior 
"55";  see  Masochistic  "55"  HI  behavior 
"66";  see  Dependent  "66"  JK  behavior 
"77";  see  Overconventional    "77"     LM 

behavior 
"88";  see  Hypemormal    "88"    NO    be- 
havior 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 


513 


autocratic;  see  Autocratic  "11"  AP  be- 
havior 

"buffoon,"  284 

classification  of  traits 
compass  of  motives,  73 
fourfold,  71 

paradigm  of  motivational  process,  73 
two-dimensional,  73-74 

conceptual  unit  of,  33  ff. 

cultural  factors  of,  22 

dependent;  see  Dependent  "'66"  JK  be- 
havior 

diagnosis,  see  Diagnosis 

dimension  of 

interpersonal,  90-238 
variability,  240-61 

distrustful;    see    Distrustful    "44"    FG 
behavior 

docile;  see  Dependent  "66"  JK  behavior 

Freudian  concept  of,  7-12,  71-72,  86 

functional  theory  of,  52 

group,  353,  426-27 

nypemormal;    see    Hypemormal    "88" 
NO  behavior 

levels   of,   75  ff.,   177,   486-81;  see   also 
Levels 

masochistic;   see   Masochistic   "55"   HI 
behavior 

multilevel  nature  of,  40  ff.,  241  ff.,  265 

narcissistic;  see  Narcissistic  "22"  BC  be- 
havior 

neurotic,  7,  442-54 

overconventional;  see  Overconventional 
"77"  LM  behavior 

profiles,  212-13,  481,  485-86,  492 

psychology,  13 

sadisuc;  see  Sadistic  "33"  DE  behavior 

schizoid,  268-81 

selection   of  variables,   38-39;  see  also 
Variables 

structure,  84-86 

Sulhvan's  definition  of,  8-9 
Personnel  Manager,  the,  405,  408, 
424-25 

seen  by  General  Manager,  424 

seen  by  Production  Manager,  424 

seen  by  Sales  Manager,  410,  413,  418, 
424 

sees  General  Manager,  423 

sees  Production  Manager,  423 

sees  Sales  Manager,  409,  423 
Phobia,  294;  see  also  Dependent  "66"  JK 

behavior 
Phobic,  180,  181,  229,  233,  235 

personality,  238,  293,  297,  300-2,  481 
Plasticity  of  human  being,  13 
Poignant  Romantic,  the,  363-65 
Power-passivity  axis,  178 


"Preconscious,"   154-55,   161  ff.,  216,  224, 
248,  306,  358,  362,  363 

and  Level  IV,  192 

and  Levels,  166,  186 

and  prediction,  167,  362  ff.,  472  ff. 

and  psychosomatic   research,    386,   387, 
389,  390,  393,  400 

and  symbolic  themes,  183-84 

and  therapy  group,  431,  433-35 

measurement  of,  464-79 
"Preconscious"  devaluation,  256 

cross-sex,  256 

hero,  256 

maternal,  256 

other,  256 

paternal,  256 
"Preconscious"  disidendfication 

cross-sex,  255 

maternal,  255 

paternal,  255 

total,  255 
"Preconscious"  duplication,  254 
"Preconscious"  fusion,  252 
"Preconscious"  idealization,  252 

cross-sex,  256 

hero,  256 

maternal,  256 

other,  256 

paternal,  256 
"Preconscious"    identification,    252,    284, 
431,  489 

cross-sex,  255 

maternal,  255 

paternal,  255 

total,  255 
"Preconscious"  repression,  254 
Prediction;  see  also  Probability 

and  "preconscious,"  167,  362  ff.,  472  ff. 

and  symbols,  \65-66 

at  Level  I-M,  108-9,  439-44 

at  Level  I-P,  106,  108-9,  426-30, 
444—45 

at  Level  III-T,  472  ff. 

dominance-submission,  indices  of,  440- 
43,  445 

love-hate,  indices  of,  440-43,  445 

of  behavior,  37,  45,  52  ff.,  108-9,  159, 
165-66,  211-16,  490 
psychotic,  362  ff. 

of  group  resistance,  426-27,  428-29 
Presenting  operations,  217-21,  228 
Private  symbolization;  see  Level  III 
Probabilistic  knowledge,  46 
Probability;  see  also  Predictioa 

and  overconventional  behavior,  305 

in  interpersonal  reflexes,  123,  125 

laws  in  conscious  communication,  139, 
141,  142 


5H 

Vrohihihty— Continued 
of  predictive  accuracy,  48,   145 
statements,  45,  50  ff.,  210,  212-13,  284 
Production  Manager,  the,  405,  407-8,  410, 
421-24 
seen  by  Sales  Manager,  410,  413 
sees  General  Manager,  421-22 
sees  Personnel  Manager,  421-22 
sees  Sales  Manager,  409,  410,  421-22 
Productive  character  type,  8 
Projection,  143,  145,  431 
Protocol  statements,  4,  34  ff .,  63 
Prototaxic  experience,  9 
Pruritis;  see  Dermatitis,  self-inflicted 
Psoriasis;    see    Dermatitis,    overtly    neu- 
rotic 
Psychasthenic,  232 
Psychiatric 
clinic  admissions  sample,  129,  152,  190, 
280-81,     290-91,     299-300,     312-13, 
321-22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50,  374, 
378-79 
and  psychosomatic  research,  374  ff. 
at  Level  I,  379-81 
at  Level  II,  381-84 
at  Level  III,  384-86 
multilevel  pattern,  397-98 
clinic  diagnosis,  443 
diagnosis  and  terms,  207-8,  229-38,  274- 
76.    286-87,    294-98,    306-10,    317-20, 
325-27,  327-29,  335-38,  345,  347-48 
hospital,  354-55 
theory,  17 
Psychologist,  in  top-management  group, 

410,  419,  420 
Psychology 
ego,  193 
industrial,  403-4 
Psychopathic,  238,  279,  300 
behavior,  348,  349-50;  see  also  Sadistic 

"33"  DE  behavior 
personality,  56,  298 
Psychosomatic,  209,  233,  272,  290,  298 
description  of  samples,  374-78 
at  Level  I-M,  379-81 
at  Level  II-C,  381-84 
at  Level  III-T,  384-86 
clinical  implications,  386  ff. 
multilevel,  386  ff. 
diagnosis,  54 
disease,  289,  319 
medicine,  376 
organ  neurosis,  298,  318 
research,  373  ff. 
Psychotic  group  behavior,  361  flf. 
Psychotic  pattern,  358-60 
implications,  359-60 
multilevel,  35&-90 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

Psychotic  samples 
at  Level  I,  356-57,  359-60,  379-81,  400-1 
at  Level  H,  357,  381-84,  400-1 
at  Level  III,  357-58,  384-86,  400-1 
clinic,  356 
private    and    State    hospital,    129,    152, 

190,  280-81,  291,  299-300,  312-13,  321- 

22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50,  374,  378 
private  hospital,   356 
State  hospital,  355 
Psychotics,  300,  320,  378,  400-2 
and  psychosomatic  research,  374  ff. 

clinical  implications,  401-2 

multilevel  pattern,  400-1 
case  histories  of,  361 

Avenger,  the,  371-72 

Commentator,  the,  369-70 

Cynic  and  Tough  Guy,  the,  367h59 

Nice  Guy,  the,  365-67 

Poignant  Romantic,  the,  363-65 

Wanderer,  the,  361 
interpersonal  diagnosis  of,  354-57 
severe,  355 
Public  communicauon;  see  Level  I 

Raters;  see  also  Level  I-S 
clinicians  as,  113-15,  212-15 
of  Thematic  Apperception  Test,  465-66 

guide  for,  466-70 
psychologists  as,  112-13,  214-16 
technicians  as,  114-15,  211-12,  214 
Rebellious  behavior;  see  Distrustful  "44" 

FG  behavior 
Receptive  character  type,  8 
Record  booklet 

for  interpersonal  analysis  of  group  dy- 
namics, 411-17 
for  interpersonal  diagnosis  of  person- 
ality, 482-88 
Relationship  principle,  176  ff. 
Relationship  statements,  139,  241-46 
Relationships 
interlevel,  241,  249  ff. 
network  of,  409-25 

reciprocal,  120  ff.,  252-56,  see  also  Inter- 
personal reflex 
and  autocratic  behavior,  65,  325 
and  dependent  behavior,  65,  293-94 
and  distrustful  behavior,  65,  270,  272- 

73 
and  hypemormal   behavior,   65,    315, 

317 
and  masochistic  behavior,  65,  284-86 
and  narcissistic  behavior,  65,  334 
and  overconventional    behavior,    65, 

304-6 
and  probability,  123,  125,  284 
and  sadistic  behavior,  65,  343-45 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 


515 


and  sado-masochistic,   344 

effect  of  "other,"  126 

individual  variation,  127-28 

multilevel,  128-30 

multilevel  patterns,  128-30,  431-36 

principle  of,  123 

qualifications  of,  128 

self-determination,   123 
symbiotic,  325,  344-45,  403,  420 

marriage    partnership,    95,    110,    126, 
128,  130 
Repression,  248-49,  252,  489 

"preconscious,"  254 
Responsible   behavior;  see  Hypernormal 

"88"  NO  behavior 
Reversal  theory  of  symbols,  156-57,  192 
Role  coincidence,  252,  254 
Role    reciprocity,    254,    405-25,    427-28, 

431-36 
Rorschach  test,  158-60,  276 

Sadism,  neurotic  mechanism,    8,128,  130 
Sadistic   "33"   DE  behavior,   50,   65,   111, 
117,  120,  130,  135,  139,  142,  194,  197, 
219-20,  231,  233,  235-38,  341;  see  also 
Psychopathic 
and  psychosomatic  research  384  ff. 
and  psychotics,  360  ff. 
and  therapy  group,  428-2P,  430,  432 
and  top  management,  412,  422,  423 
at  Level  I,  93,  95-96,  104-5 
at  Level  III,  170  ff.,  372,  384 
check-list  items,  456 
clinical  manifestations  of,  345 
effect  of,  343^5 
in  Death  of  a  Salesman,  104 
in  Freud's  "Irma"  dream,  172 
in  nursery  school  situation,  105 
in  "The  Secret  Life  of  Walter  Mitty, 

173-74 
in  typical  TAT  themes,  175-76, 

466-70 
incidence  in  cultural  samples,  129,  152, 

190,  349-50 
psychopathic  behavior,  347-48 
purpose  of,  342-43 
Sado-masochism,  130,  288-89,  344-45,  403 
Sado-masochistic  conflict,  181 
Sales  Manager,  the,  405,  408,  409-18 
sees  General  Manager,  410,  412 
sees  Personnel  Manager,  410,  412 
sees  Production  Manager,  410,  412 
Samples 
clinic  psychotic,  356 
college   undergraduate,    129,    280,    290, 

299,  312,  321,  330,  338,  349 
dermatitis,  376-77,  458 
neuro-,  290 


overtly  neurotic,  129,  130,  152,   190, 
280-81,  290-91,  299-300,  312-13,  321- 
22,   330-31,    338,   340,    349-50,   374, 
379  ff. 
self-inflicted,  and  unanxious,  129,  130, 
152,  190,  280-81,  291,  299-300,  312- 
13,  321-22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50, 
374,  379  ff. 
group  psychotherapy,  129,  130,  152,  190, 
280-81,  291,  299-300,  312-13,   321-22, 
330-31,  338,  340,  349-50,  426-36 
hospitalized    psychotic,    129,    152,    190, 
280-81,   291,  299-300,   312-13,   321- 
22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50,  374  ff. 
private-,  356 
State-,  355 
hypertensive,  129,  152,  190,  280-81,  291, 
299-300,   312-13,  321-22,  330-31,   338, 
340,  349-50,  374,  375,  379  ff. 
individual  psychotherapy,  129,  130,  152, 
190,  280-81,  291,  299-300,  312-13,  321- 
22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50 
Interpersonal    Check    List    correlation, 

462 
MMPI  standardization  441 
neurotic,  374,  379  ff. 
normal  (medical)  control,  129,  152,  190, 
280-81,   291,   299-300,   312-13,   321- 
22,    330-31,    338,    340,    349-50,   374, 
377,  379  ff. 
multilevel  diagnosis  of,  381 
normative,  494-99 

obese    middle-class    females,    129,    152, 
190,  280-81,  290-91,  299-300,  312-13, 
321-22,    330-31,    338,    340,    349-50, 
374,  375,  379ff.,  458,  461-62 
as  control,  472  ff. 
officers   in    military   service,    129,    130, 
280,    291,    299,    312,    321,    330,    338, 
349 
psychiatric  clinic  admissions,  129,  152, 
190,  280-81,  290-91,  299-300,   312-13, 
321-22,  330-31,  338,  340,  349-50,  374, 
379  ff.,  463,  493 
stockade  prisoners,  129,   130,  280,  291, 

299,  312,  321,  330,  338,  349 
TAT  standardization,  472-78 
top-management  group,  403-25 

ulcer,    129,    152,   190,   280-81,  291,  299- 

300,  312-13,  321-22,  330-31,  338,  340, 
349-50,  374-75,  379 ff. 

university,  458 

counseling    center,    males,    129,    280, 

291,  299,  312,  321,  330,  338,  349 
graduate    students,    males,    129,    280, 

291,  299,  312,  321,  330,  338,  349 
psychiatric  clinic,  129,  280,  290,  299, 

312,  321,  330,  338,  349 


5i6 

Schizoid,  48,  56,  57,  124,  138-J9.  229,  233, 
235,  238,  267-68,  276-77;  see  also  Dis- 
trustful "44"  FG  behavior 
and  obsessive  behavior,  278-79,  280-81, 
289,  298,  308,  310-11 
Schizophrenia,  55,  278,  355;  see  also  Dis- 
trustful "44"  FG  behavior 
Schizophrenic,   207 
Science,  general 
aims,  of,  50-52 
language  of,  35-38 

operational  definitions  of  terms,  47-48 
probabilistic  knowledge,  46 
social  criterion  of  knowledge,  45 
validity  of  empirical  knowledge,  48-49 
Seborrheic     dermatitis;     see    Dermatitis, 

overtly  neurotic 
Security  operations,  10,  15;  see  also  Anx- 
iety; Interpersonal  reflex 
at  Level  I,  91-131 
at  Level  11,  132-53,  492 
at  Level  III,  154-91 
at  Level  IV,  192-99 
at  Level  V,  200-6 
autocratic,  323-31 
dependent,  292-302 
distrustful,  269-81 
group,  427 
hypemormal,  315-22 
masochistic,  282-91 
effect  of,  284-86 
narcissistic,  332-40 
overconventional,  303-14 
sadistic,  341-50 
Self-acceptance,   205,   252,   255;   see   also 

Self-rejection 
Self-deception,  254,  4S6;  see  also  Misper- 
ception 
and  management  group,  406  ff. 
Self -depreciation;  see  Masochistic  "55"  HI 

behavior 
Self-determination 
locus  of  responsibility,  18-24 
principle  of,  115-118,  122 
Self-effacement;  see  Masochistic  "55"  HI 

behavior 
Self-inflicted    dermatitis,    see   Dermatitis, 

self-inflicted 
Self-perception,  254,  431;  see  also  Self- 
deception 
Self-rejection,  255 
"Self-righteous  moralist,"  337-38 
Social  behaviorism,  99 
Social  criterion  of  knowledge,  45;  see  also 
Language,  scientific,  empirical  prop- 
ositions 
Sociometrics,    403-25,    429  ff.,    see    also 
Level  I-S 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

and  Interpersonal  Check  List,  112,  458 
Level  I,  78,  81,  85,  102,  106,  443-54 
record  booklet,  409 
Standard  scores,  489,  493-99 
Submission;  see  Dominance-submission 
Superego,  201 

Symbiotic  relationships;  see  Relation- 
ships 
Symbolic  expressions,  154 
Symbolic  function,  155,  156 
Symbolic  hero,  166,  119 
Symbolic  life,  155 
Symbolic  others,  119 
Symbolic  self,  168 
Symbolic  themes  and  "preconscious," 

183-84 
Symbolic  world,  166 
Symptom,  24,  142,  490-92 
and  character,  24-26 
autocratic,  324  ff. 
alcoholism,  326 
compulsive,  327-29 
impotence,   327 
manic,  324 
obsessive,  328-29 
dependent,  294-300 
anxiety  hysteria,  297-98 
anxiety  neurosis,  296-97 
hypochondriacal,  295,  297-98 
neurasthenic,  297 
phobic,  294-98 
physical,  295-96 
psychosomatic,  298 
dermatological,  376-77 
diagnosis  of,  235-37,  443 
distrustful,  274  ff. 
flat  affect,  274 
schizoid  psychosis,  277-78 
hypemormal,  3 16  ff . 
frigidity  and,  319 
impotence  and,  319 
organ  neurosis,  318 
paranoia,  319 
masochistic,  286  ff. 
compulsive,  287 
obsessive,  287 

sado-,  130,  181,  288-89,  344-45,  403 
narcissistic,  332  ff. 

psychosomatic,  335-36 
overconventional,  306  ff. 
anxiety  and,  307 
complaints  of  others'  behavior  and, 

307-10 
frigidity  and,  308 
hysterical,  310-14 
physical,  307-8 
psychosomatic,  298,  374  ff. 
psychotic,  356  ff. 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

sadistic,  345-50 

ulcer,  375 
Symptomatic  behavior,  106 
Symptomatic  conditions,  159 
Symptomatic  indices,  439-44 
Symptomatic  pressure,  443 
Syntaxic  experience,  9,  30 

Teaching 
interpersonal  mechanism,  323-24 
reflex,  94 
Temporal  variabihty,  75 
Tests,  276;  see  also  Iflund  projective  test; 
Interpersonal  Check  List;  Level  I-M, 
I-P,  I-R,  I-S,  I-T;  Level  II-C;  III-B, 
Ill-i,    III-IFT,    III-M;    Level    III-T, 
Level  V-C;  TAT;  etc. 
and  psychotics,  359-60 
development    of    Interpersonal    Check 

List,  457-63 
interpersonal  battery,  115,  210,  439 
projective,  158-60,  167 
situation,  106 
Thematic  Apperception  Test,  114-15,  158, 
171,   184-86,  197-98,  464-79;  see  also 
Level  III-T;  Level  III,  Hero,  Other 
and  psychotics,  360 
cards  used  in  interpersonal  system, 

464 
content  interpretauon,  198 
designating  the  hero,  465 
guide  for  rating,  466-70 
interpersonal  formulae,  69 
norms,  472,  496-97 
scoring,  175-79,  465-72 
molar,    176,  470-71 
molecular,  175 
validation  sample,  472-78 
Therapy  group;  see  Group  therapy 
Time-binding,  164-65 
Top  management;  see  Management  group 
"Transference,"   142-43,   171 
Two-dimensional    behavior,    representa- 
tion of,  73 
Two-dimensional  surface,  74 

Ulcer  patient 
and  psychosomatic  research,  374  ff. 

at  Level  I,  379-81 

at  Level  II,  381-84 

at  Level  III,  384-86 

clinical  implications,  389-90 

multilevel  pattern,  388-89 
interpersonal  reflexes  of,  445-53 
MMPI  profile  of,  442-43 
samples,  129,  152,  190,  280-81,  291,  299- 

300,  312-13,  321-22,  330-31,  338,  340, 

349-50,  374-75 


Unconscious 

Freud,  10 

Jung,  20,  22 

Sulbvan,  9 

unexpressed;  see  Level  IV 
Underlying  operations,  217-27 
Unexpressed  unconscious,  see  Level  IV 
Urticaria;  see  Dermatitis,  unanxious 

Value  systems 
function  of,  201 
universality  of,  201 
Variability;  see  also  Variables 
and  sublevels  of  consciousness, 

148-51 
diagnosis  norms,  494-99 
dimension,  242-43 
compared  with  interpersonal. 
244-46 
indices,  85-86,  241-61,  246,  248,  486-81, 
489,    494-99,    see    also    Conscious 
disidentification,   -fusion,  -idealiza- 
tion, -identification,  Cross-level  dif- 
fusion,   -disidentification,   -identifi- 
cation; Devaluation;  Diffusion;  Dis- 
equation;  Equation;  Fusion,  Identi- 
tihcation;  Interpersonal  perception; 
Misperception;      Other-mispercep- 
tion;    Other-perception;    "Precon- 
scious"    devaluation,    -disidentifica- 
tion, -duplication,  -fusion,  -ideali- 
zation,   -identification,   -repression; 
Repression;  Role  coincidence,  -rec- 
iprocity;     Self-acceptance;      Self- 
deception;     Self-perception;     Self- 
rejection 
and  interlevel  conflict,  241 
codes,  253-56 
key  to,  256 
listed,  252,  254-56 
function  of,  247-48 
in  group  dynamics,  414-15 
measurement  of,  257-61 
misperception   illustrated,   410 
operational  definition  of,  257 
generic,  251-53 
specific,  253-56 
schematized,  250 
structural,  251 
temporal,  257 
verbal  summaries,  of,  488 
interpretations  of,  248-49 
of  interpersonal  behavior,  75 
of  symbols,  157 
profiles,  212 
situational,  75,  243-44 
structural,  75,  243,  479 
temporal,  243-44,  479 


5i8 

Variables,  26 
interpersonal,  127-28,  220,  241-46 

adaptive,  220,  247-48 

at  Level  I,  441-43 

at  Level  III,  157,  170-71 

classification  of,  65,  13S 

defensive,  247-48 

development  of,  62  ff .,  245-46 

intensity  of,  66 

maladaptive,  220 

selection  of,  38-39,  103  ff. 

systematic  relatedness,  39,  64-66 
intervariable  correlation,  461-62 
intrapersonal,   127-28,  244-46,  see  also 
Variability,  indices 

and  projection,  244 

and  suppression,  244 
multilevel  relatedness  of,  43 


INDEX  OF  SUBJECTS 

noninterpersonal,  159-60 
personality,  selection  of,  38-39 
"therapeutically  relevant,"  208 

Wanderer,  the,  361-63 

Warts;  see  Dermatitis,  unanxious 

Working  Principles 

I,  15-6 

II,  26 

III,  39 

IV,  39 

V,  40 

VI,  42 

VII,  43 

VIII,  45 

IX,  58 

I  through  IX,  59-60 


In  the  decade  before  he  became  the  highly  controversial  director  of 
psychedelic  drug  research  at  Harvard,  Timothy  Leary  was  one  of  the 
leading  clinical  psychologists  practicing  in  the  U.S.,  heading  the 
prestigious  Kaiser  Foundation  Psychological  Research  Center  in  Oakland. 

INTERPERSONAL  DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY  (1957),  his  first 
full-length  book,  summarizes  the  innovative  experimental  studies  in 
interpersonal  behavior  performed  by  the  author  and  his  associates  at  the 
Kaiser  Foundation  and  in  private  practice  between  1950  and  1957. 


".  .  .  perhaps  the  most  important  clinical  book  to  appear  this  year....  Rarely  has 
psychology  found  a  way  of  placing  so  many  different  data  into  the  same  schematic 
system,  and  the  implications  of  this  are  potentially  breathtaking  " 

-  ANNUAL  REVIEW  OF  PSYCHOLOGY  (1958) 

"Leary 's  enduring  contribution  to  psychodiagnosis,  or,  more  generally,  to  the  typology 
of  personality,  is  embodied  in  his  honored  1957  volume,  INTERPERSONAL 
DIAGNOSIS  OF  PERSONALITY.  .  .  The  concept  of  levels  was  implicit  in 
sophisticated  personality  descriptions,  and  degrees  of  consciousness  were  recognized  in 
all  the  psychodynamically  based  systems,  but  none  were  connected  systematically 
through  the  concept  of  interpersonal  behavior  as  in  the  Leary  system.  " 

-  Frank  Barron 

author  of  numerous  books  on  the  psychology  of  creativity 


Dr.  Timothy  Leary,  PhD  (1920-1996)  Psychologist,  philosopher,  explorer, 
teacher,  optimist,  author  and  revolutionary  avatar  of  the  mind.  Often  called  the 
Galileo  of  Consciousness,  he  went  public  with  his  observations  of  the  mind 
made  with  psychedelic  mindscopes  and  helped  initiate  a  renaissance  which  is 
still  only  beginning  to  elaborate  itself 

ISBN  l-S12MM-77b-7 

Cover  Design  by  Matthew  Stock 


Resource  Publications 

An  imprint  of  Wipf  and  Stock  Publishers 
199  West  8th  Avenue  •  Eugene  OR  97401 


% 


781592'l447763