Skip to main content

Full text of "THE MENTAL HEALTH OF THE CHILD"

See other formats


Julius  Segal,  Ph.D.,  Editor 

Authors: 

Antoinette  Gattozzi 
Gay  Luce 
Maya  Pines 
Clarissa  Wittenberg 
Herbert  Yahraes 


U&SUPT.OFDOC& 


Program  Analysis  and  Evaluation  Branch 
Office  of  Program  Planning  and  Evaluation 
National  Institute  of  Mental  Health 
5600  Fishers  Lane 
Rockville,  Md.  20852 

June  1971 


Public  Health  Service  publication  No.  2168 
Printed  1971 


For  skle  by  the  Superintendent  of  Documents,  U.S.  Government  Printing  Office 

Washington,  D  C  20402  -  Price  $5 

Stock  Number  1724-0132 


Page 

FOREWORD v 

PREFATORY  NOTE    ix 

STUDIES  AND  DEMONSTRATIONS  IN  PREVENTION  i 

Nursery  Schools  in  the  Service  of  Mental  Health 3 

Reducing  the  Effects  of  Cultural  Deprivation  25 

A  Pre-school  Program  for  Disadvantaged  Children 44 

Baker's  Dozen:  A  Program  for  Training  Young  People  as 

Mental  Health  Aides  59 

Behavioral  Consequences  of  Alienation 72 

Alternatives  to  Violence  86 

Child  Development  Counselors:  Lessons  From  Their  Training 

and  Use  104 

Operation  Hope:  Educating  New  Leaders 113 

FACTORS    THAT    INFLUENCE    THE    CHILD'S    MENTAL 

HEALTH  129 

Childhood  Influences  on  Intelligence,  Personality,  and  Mental 

Health  131 

An  Anthropological  Investigation  of  Child-Rearing  Practices 

and  Adult  Personality  155 

The  Effects  of  Early  Experience  on  a  Child's  Development  .  178 

Determinants  of  Mother-Infant  Interaction 193 

Studying  the  Mother-Infant  Relationship  for  Clues  to  the 

Causes  of  Aberrant  Development 200 

How  the  Child  Separates  From  the  Mother 211 

Early  Social  Development  in  Children 222 

Family  Communication  and  Child  Development 235 

Young  People  of  Normal  Mental  Health  240 

The  Impact  of  Visual  Media  on  Personality 247 

Parental  Behavior  and  the  Origins  of  Schizophrenia 267 

The  Causes  and  Treatment  of  Childhood  Schizophrenia  .  .  ,  293 
Schizophrenia:  New  Light  from  the  Life  Histories  and 

Biochemistry  of  Siblings  and  Twins     310 


in 


Page 

ADVANCES  IN  DIAGNOSIS  AND  TREATMENT  327 

Why  Adolescents  Kill  Themselves 329 

Studies  of  Child  Abuse  and  Infant  Accidents 343 

The  Re-Education  of  Criminals     371 

Delinquent  Gangs'  An  Answer  to  the  Needs  of  the  Socially 

Disabled 384 

Delinquent  Girl  Gangs 395 

Psychiatric  Drugs  for  Children 398 

Brief  Psychotherapy  vs.  Drugs:  Fitting  the  Treatment  to  the 

Illness 401 

New  Approaches  to  the  Treatment  of  Very  Young  Schizo- 
phrenic Children 412 

An  Experiment  in  Foster  Care  for  Seriously  Disturbed  Boys  .  429 

The  Role  of  Learning  in  Relapse  of  Narcotic  Addicts 440 

Psychodynamics  of  Asthmatic  Children 450 

Infant  Stimulation   As  Part  of  Well  Baby  Care  in  A  Dis- 

advantaged  Area     463 

BASIC  STUDIES  OF  CHILD  DEVELOPMENT  477 

The  Development  of  Intelligence  in  Babies 479 

How  Children  Learn  English 491 

Experiments  in  How  We  Learn  to  Coordinate    495 

Heredity's  Effect  on  Behavior  Under  Stress     503 

Genetics  of  Human  Behavior 512 

Patterns  of  Sleep  Over  a  Lifetime    523 

The  Physiological  Imprint  of  Learning     539 

Biological  Bases  of  Memory     557 

Hormones  in  the  Development  of  Behavior 567 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS    588 


In  every  age,  men  have  recognized  the  special  importance  of  the  child's 
role  in  society.  Whether  through  ancient  tribal  initiation  rituals  or  the 
pronouncements  of  contemporaiy  psychoanalytic  theorists,  each  succeed- 
ing civilization  has  acknowledged  that  the  child  will  cany  after  us  the 
imprint  of  the  world  we  create  around  him.  Paradoxically,  however,  the 
child  has  also  been  a  traditional  victim  of  our  neglect  and  abuse.  An 
inability  and  unwillingness  to  respond  to  the  special  needs  of  the  child 
haunt  us  in  the  ghettos  and  suburbs  of  twentieth-century  America  as  they 
did  in  the  factory  towns  of  the  industrial  revolution.  Tonight,  thousands 
of  American  children  will  fall  into  a  troubled  sleep,  bearing  the  scars  of 
emotional  tiauma  and  physical  pain  inflicted  by  adults  no  less  misguided 
and  ill  than  those  who  nurtuied  the  cruel  children's  prisons  of  eighteenth- 
century  England.  We  have  not  yet  matched  our  best  instincts  and  insights 
with  comparable  action— and  nowhere  is  this  more  apparent  than  in  the 
field  of  mental  health. 

Over  one  half  of  the  U.S.  population  is  now  under  25  years  of  age,  and 
it  is  estimated  that  over  10  percent  of  this  precious  resomce— about  ten 
million  of  oui  youth-require  mental  health  services.  Their  needs  range 
from  hospitalization  to  treat  and  reverse  serious  psychopathology,  to  the 
early  interventions  of  doctors,  teachers,  counselors,  and  parents  able  to 
handle  mild,  transient  problems  and  thus  prevent  later  developmental 
crises. 

The  child's  mental  health  needs  have  challenged  our  best  efforts  to 
date.  In  the  State  mental  hospitals  of  our  Nation  where,  for  the  past 
fifteen  years,  the  number  of  adult  patients  has  been  declining  steadily, 
both  the  first  admission  and  the  resident  population  rates  for  children 
have  increased  at  an  accelerated  pace.  While  many  children  are  receiving 
the  mental  health  care  they  require-some  52,000  in  community  mental 
health  centers,  33,000  in  public  and  private  mental  hospitals,  26,000  in 
residential  treatment  centers,  and  526,000  in  psychiatric  outpatient 
clinics-millions  more  are  going  without  help.  Of  the  Nation's  ap- 
proximately 2,300  mental  health  clinics  in  1968,  somewhat  less  than  one 
tenth  were  child  guidance  clinics;  moreover,  only  40  percent  of  the 
268,000  patients  under  18  years  seen  at  such  clinics  were  actually  treated, 
the  remaining  60  percent  receiving  no  more  than  a  diagnosis.  A  large 
proportion  of  all  counties  in  the  United  States  are  without  mental  health 
clinics  altogether,  and  most  of  these  also  lack  agencies  that  substitute  in 
some  measure  for  such  services.  The  cost  to  society— in  wasted  resources 
and  human  suffering-can  only  be  guessed.  But  in  1969  nearly  one  million 
children  aged  10  to  17  were  brought  before  juvenile  courts,  and  during  the 
past  two  decades  the  suicide  rate  among  adolescents  and  young  adults  has 
increased  by  60  percent. 


These  data  do  not  minimize  the  success  of  efforts  to  date  conducted  or 
supported  by  the  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health.  From  its  very  incep- 
tion, NIMH  has  devoted  a  large  part  of  its  program  to  activities  relating  to 
the  mental  health  of  children,  many  of  them  reflected  in  this  report. 
Contributing  to  the  overall  effort  today  is  the  entire  range  of  the  Insti- 
tute's resources. 

•  Community  mental  health  center  programs  now  not  only  provide 
mental  health  services  directly  to  troubled  children  and  their  families,  but 
also  serve  as  a  rich  vehicle  for  acquainting  a  variety  of  community  agencies 
with  mental  health  principles  and  practices,  thereby  enabling  them  better 
to  foster  the  well-being  of  our  young.  In  nearly  two  thirds  of  the  com- 
munity mental  health   centers   funded  to  date,  specialized  services  to 
children  are  highlighted. 

•  Projects   designed  to  improve  the  Nation's  mental  hospitals  have 
focused  heavily  on  upgrading  the  quality  of  institutional  care  provided  to 
seriously  disturbed  children. 

•  Interdisciplinary  training  programs  directed  at  developing  skilled  child 
therapists  have  been  expanded,  many  of  them  emphasizing  the  prevention 
of  child  behavior  disorders.   Such  programs  have  led  to  a  significant 
increase  in  the  number  of  professionals  and  paraprofessionals  qualified  to 
work  with  children  and  their  families  in  both  preventive  and  remedial 
activities. 

•  New  light  on  the  dynamics  of  child  development— and  on  our  ability 
to  influence  its  course— has  been  shed  by  scientists  devoted  to  child  re- 
search. Our  understanding  of  the  complex  biological,  psychological  and 
social  factors  involved  in  mental  illness  has  considerably  increased;  we  are 
better  able  to  detect  incipient  emotional  trouble  and  therefore  to  take 
preventive  and  curative  measures  early,  when  they  are  most  likely  to  be 
effective.   A   number  of  promising  approaches  have  been   tested   and 
demonstrated  for  building  in  the  very  young  a  strong  foundation  for  both 
emotional  health  and  intellectual  competence. 

•  And,  finally,  we  now  have  more  hopeful  treatments  for  the  whole 
range    of  childhood   disorders—from   problem   behavior   to    childhood 
schizophrenia. 

Despite  such  advances,  much  more  must  be  done.  Activities  directed  at 
improving  the  mental  health  of  children  now  carry  the  highest  priority  for 
NIMH.  Our  intent  is  to  provide  a  base  of  knowledge,  techniques,  man- 
power, and  services  that  will  not  only  reduce  significantly  the  number  of 
our  mentally  and  emotionally  ill  children,  but  also  enhance  the  well-being 
and  productivity  of  all  our  youth. 

This  volume,  issued  as  the  Institute  marks  the  25th  anniversary  of  the 
National  Mental  Health  Act,  is  intended  to  display  the  range  of  the  Insti- 
tute's current  and  past  efforts  in  the  child  mental  health  field.  Rather  than 
an  exercise  in  self-celebration,  the  report  serves  as  a  reminder  of  the 
beginnings  that  have  been  made-and  of  what  more  we  can  do.  It  is 
intended  to  stimulate  further  the  creative  approaches  of  research  person- 
nel, and  to  provide  information  of  value  to  clinicians  and  all  those 


others  who  deal  directly  with  the  child.  A  number  of  studies  and  demon- 
strations reported  here  may  well  serve  as  models  for  future  efforts  else- 
where. 

Highlighting  its  importance,  the  area  of  prevention  is  the  focus  of  the 
first  section  of  this  volume.  Emphasis  here  is  on  the  child  in  his  normal 
environment— the  family,  the  school,  the  community—where  the  task  is  to 
so  enhance  the  child's  environment  that  mental  health  is  maintained  and 
pathology  is  aborted  at  the  primary  source,  before  the  child  must  be 
separated  from  the  general  population.  A  number  of  NIMH  efforts  in  this 
direction  are  reported  here— including  attempts  to  demonstrate  methods 
for  enriching  the  intellectual  and  emotional  world  of  deprived  and 
minority  children  and  to  show  how  the  corrosive  effects  of  prejudice  can 
be  prevented  early  in  the  life  of  the  child.  Another  goal  is  to  help  the 
older  disadvantaged  youth,  often  troubled  and  embittered,  to  work  for  his 
dreams  and  realize  his  capabilities.  One  team  of  investigators  has 
provided  dramatic  evidence  that  worldly  wise,  angry  young  people  in  the 
ghetto  need  not  live  without  hope;  properly  motivated  and  placed  in  a 
college  setting,  they  demonstrate  untapped  depths  of  wisdom  and  ability. 

The  second  section  of  this  volume  is  concerned  with  a  variety  of 
circumstances  that  can  affect  a  child's  mental  health-and  thus  relates  not 
only  to  the  etiology  of  behavior  problems,  but  to  their  prevention  as  well. 
Reported  here  are  a  number  of  the  Institute's  efforts  to  learn  precisely 
which  factors  in  early  life  either  enhance  or  reduce  the  potential  for 
intellectual  growth  and  emotional  well-being.  These  projects  include,  for 
example,  detailed  observations  of  the  effects  of  various  relationships 
existing  between  infants  and  mothers,  as  well  as  long-range  studies  of  the 
connection  between  certain  characteristics  of  parents  and  the  psycho- 
logical development  of  their  children,  One  study  is  concerned  with  the 
influence  of  visual  media-in  particular,  of  television-on  childhood 
behavior,  while  another  deals  with  child-rearing  practices  and  outcomes  in 
societies  other  than  our  own.  The  section  ends  with  three  studies  of 
factors  contributing  to  schizophrenia— the  most  serious  and  devastating 
form  of  mental  illness,  which  so  often  and  so  cruelly  afflicts  young 
people. 

The  third  section  presents  an  array  of  projects  concerned  both  with 
improving  the  diagnosis  and  treatment  of  disturbed  children  and 
adolescents,  and  with  increasing  our  understanding  of  some  of  the  con- 
ditions leading  to  disturbed  behavior.  The  studies  reported  here  deal  with 
a  broad  range  of  problem  children-including  those  diagnosed  as 
schizophrenic,  neurotic,  and  hyperkinetic,  as  well  as  seriously  disturbed 
boys,  members  of  delinquent  gangs,  narcotic  addicts,  young  criminals,  and 
suicidal  adolescents.  The  range  of  efforts  in  their  behalf  is  also  very  broad, 
embracing,  for  example,  behavior  therapy,  brief  psychotherapy, 
psychiatric  drugs,  a  foster  home  program,  hospitalization  and  counseling 
for  potential  suicides.  The  children  of  poor,  unmarried,  teenage  girls,  one 
recent  study  shows,  need  not  be  victimized  by  the  nature  of  their 
beginnings-nor  are  the  mothers  themselves  without  the  capacity  for 
psychological  growth  and  maturation.  Another  report  describes  attempts 

vii 


to  understand  and  deal  with  the  problem  of  child  abuse,  and  still  another 
provides  a  demonstration  of  how  young  criminals  might  be  reeducated  in 
a  prison  setting. 

The  foundation  of  all  our  efforts  in  child  mental  health  must  be  basic 
research-an  effort  to  understand  both  normal  and  abnormal  development 
and  behavior-and  the  final  section  of  this  volume  includes  reports  of 
some  of  the  Institute's  endeavors  in  this  area.  The  studies  described  here 
explore  such  fundamental  variables  as  the  biology  of  learning  and 
memory,  the  effects  of  hormones  on  behavior,  normal  and  abnoimal 
patterns  of  sleep  at  various  age-levels,  the  influence  of  heredity  on 
behavior  under  stress  and  on  intelligence  and  personality,  and  the  growth 
of  intelligence  hi  babies.  Findings  from  such  studies  considerably 
strengthen  our  ability  to  prevent  and  treat  mental  disorders  in  children. 

The  NIMH  efforts  in  the  child  mental  health  field  clearly  cannot  be 
encompassed  in  a  single  report,  for  they  involve  the  varied  activities  of 
research  scientists,  clinicians,  community  agencies,  and  training  insti- 
tutions. The  program  includes  work  in  the  most  basic  sciences— for 
example,  in  biochemistry,  genetics,  and  experimental  psychology-along 
with  clinical  and  community  studies.  The  examples  reported  here,  how- 
ever, will  help  provide  the  reader  with  an  appreciation  not  alone  of  the 
scope  and  complexity  of  the  NIMH  program  in  behalf  of  children,  but  also 
of  its  guiding  purpose.  Underlying  all  of  the  Institute's  varied  efforts- 
from  basic  research  to  community  consultations— is  the  endeavor  to  meet 
our  children's  mental  health  needs. 

In  pursuing  its  child  mental  health  program,  the  Institute  seeks  every 
opportunity  to  collaborate  with  programs  and  agencies  whose  efforts 
complement  our  own.  The  factors  affecting  the  mental  health  of  children 
are  too  vast  and  complex  to  be  dealt  with  by  any  one  organization  alone. 
Mental  health  problems  make  themselves  felt  in  virtually  every  aspect  of  a 
child's  life— at  home,  at  school,  in  the  world  of  work— and  their  solution, 
therefore,  requires  the  contributions  of  agencies  with  varied  missions  and 
programs,  each  emphasizing  different  dimensions  of  the  child's  world. 

The  mental  health  of  our  children  rests,  ultimately,  on  the  health  of  our 
total  society-from  the  smallest  unit  to  the  largest.  The  stability  of  the 
home  and  the  well-being  of  the  family,  the  compassion  of  the  surrounding 
community,  the  social  conscience  and  social  action  of  our  Government 
and  its  citizens-all  of  these  are  crucial.  The  twenty-five  years  past  arc 
viewed  with  satisfaction,  but  also  with  an  awareness  of  the  formidable 
tasks  not  yet  completed.  Our  pledge  is  to  continue  and  expand  these 
efforts,  now  well  begun,  to  improve  the  mental  health  of  children  and 
thus  to  enhance  the  quality  of  their  lives-and  our  own. 


0- 


Bertram  S.  Brown,  M.D. 

Director 
National  Institute  of  Mental  Health 


viu 


This  volume,  intended  to  provide  examples  of  past  and  current  efforts 
by  NIMH  in  the  child  mental  health  field,  includes  primarily  reports 
published  earlier  in  the  Institute's  Research  Project  Summaries  and  Pro- 
gram  Reports  series.  Seven  reports,  describing  more  recent  advances, 
appear  heie  for  the  first  time.  They  begin  on  pages  3,  44,  1 13,  310,  343, 
463,  and  479.  The  earlier  reports  are  presented  as  originally  published, 
though  in  many  cases  the  investigators  nave  done  further  work  on  their 
projects,  and  in  some  instances  the  locations  of  the  investigators,  their 
titles,  or  the  official  designation  of  their  institutions  have  changed. 

The  42  reports  included  here  are  grouped  into  four  major  areas, 
covering  prevention,  etiology,  diagnosis  and  treatment,  and  basic  research. 
To  a  considerable  extent,  however,  the  groups  overlap;  work  reported  in 
one  group,  in  a  number  of  cases,  could  have  been  included  with  equal 
justification  in  another.  It  should  be  emphasized  also  that  the  projects 
described  here  were  chosen  simply  as  representative  of  NIMH  activities  in 
the  field  of  child  mental  health.  They  do  not  deal  with  more  important 
subjects  and  they  do  not  present  more  useful  findings  than  do  scores  of 
other  Institute-supported  projects  in  the  area. 


IX 


Bitter  are  the  tears  of  a  child; 

Sweeten  them. 
Deep  are  the  thoughts  of  a  child; 

Quiet  them. 
Sharp  is  the  grief  of  a  child: 

Take  it  from  him. 
Soft  is  the  heart  of  a  child: 

Do  not  harden  it. 


Lady  Pamela  Wyndham  Glenconner 


Investigator. 

Mary  B.  Lane,  Ed.D. 

San  Francisco  State  College 

San  Francisco,  California 

Prepared  by. 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Introduction  and  Summary 


An  Institute-supported  program  in  San  Francisco  has  been  quietly 
fighting  racial  prejudice-regarded  as  a  major  mental  health  problem- 
through  the  medium  of  little  children.  In  an  area  of  the  city  known  as  the 
Western  Addition,  where  slum  neighborhoods  are  being  replaced  by  urban 
renewal  projects,  to  the  resentment  of  the  predominantly  poor  and 
predominantly  black  population,  this  program  has  demonstrated  that  a 
special  kind  of  nuisery  school  can  bring  together  all  kinds  of  people. 

Mary  B,  Lane,  professor  of  education  at  San  Francisco  State  College, 
who  conceived  the  program,  calls  this  kind  of  nursery  school  cross-cultural 
or  multi-cultural.  By  this  she  means  a  school  that  enrolls  children  of 
different  races  and  socioeconomic  levels  and  uses  special  curricular  and 
organizational  devices  to  promote  interfamily,  interrace  relations. 

In  1966,  Dr,  Lane  and  her  associates  opened  three  nursery  schools  for 
60  children,  selected  in  door-to-door  canvassing,  who  had  recently  passed 
or  soon  would  pass  their  second  birthday.  The  racial  and  socioeconomic 
background  of  the  children  reflected  those  of  the  community.  About  60 
percent  of  the  youngsters  were  black,  30  percent  white,  and  10  percent 
Oriental,  meaning  Chinese,  Japanese,  or  mixed.  About  one-third  of  them 
came  from  families  living  in  low-cost  public  housing;  one-third  of  the 
children  came  from  new,  cooperatively  owned,  apartment  development 
for  people  of  middle  income;  and  the  rest  from  families  in  individual 
dwellings.  This  last  group-designated  for  research  purposes  as  the 
"random  housing"  group-ranged  from  families  of  unskilled  workers  to 
those  of  professional  men.  In  each  racial  group,  the  families  represented 
several  socioeconomic  levels. 

The  schools  were  known  as  Nurseries  in  Cross-Cultural  Education,  or 
NICE.  They  were  in  rooms  lent  by  the  Buchanan  Y.M.C.A.,  the  Christ 
United  Presbyterian  Church,  and-in  a  public  housing  unit  named  the 
Westside  Courts— by  the  San  Francisco  Housing  Authority. 

Each  school  was  staffed  by  a  professionally  trained  nursery  school 
leader,  an  assistant  who  had  had  experience  with  children,  though  not  as  a 
teacher,  and  a  part-time  aide  who  was  from  the  community.  The  staffs 


were  tacially  mixed-white  and  black.  An  effort  to  recruit  Oriental 
teachers  failed.  One  teacher  or  aide  at  each  school  was  a  man  because  Dr. 
Lane  thought  that  the  many  children,  about  a  fourth  of  the  total,  without 
a  male  figure  in  the  home  should  have  one  at  school. 

The  general  aim  was  to  assess  the  schools  as  instruments  for  promoting 
mental  health  in  a  community  subjected  to  the  stresses  of  redevelopment. 
Results  were  to  be  appraised  in  teims  of  factors  related  to  mental  health 
such  as  basic  trust,  autonomy,  initiative,  cognitive  development,  and  social 
competence,  in  the  case  of  the  children,  and,  in  the  case  of  the  families, 
social  competence,  intergroup  acceptance,  and  utilization  of  community 
resources. 

The  program  stressed  parent  involvement.  Mothers  gradually  took  on 
the  role  of  aides  during  the  sessions-three  houis  every  morning -and 
participated  in  a  numbei  of  after-school  activities,  sometimes  with  fathers. 
During  the  second  year,  responding  to  mothers'  requests  for  more  in- 
formation about  techniques  of  teaching,  Dr.  Lane  gave  a  30-hour  training, 
course,  modelling  it  on  one  of  her  courses  at  San  Francisco  State  for 
prospective  nursery  school  teachers.  To  meet  the  demand  the  following 
year,  two  such  courses  had  to  be  given. 

During  the  second  year  also,  both  to  promote  closer  ties  between 
school  and  home  and  to  help  mothers  become  better  teachers  of  their 
children,  the  program  developed  a  series  of  "home  tasks"— things  to  be 
made,  experiments  to  be  tried,  stories  to  be  read,  games  to  be  played.  One 
of  these  was  taken  to  each  home  each  week  by  a  member  of  the  staff,  or 
by  students  from  San  Francisco  State,  and  explained  to  the  mother.  The 
following  week  it  was  picked  up  and  the  experiences  of  mother  and  child 
recorded. 

Befoie  the  start  of  the  third  year,  the  parents  formed  the  Parent 
Advisory  Council  to  help  guide  NICE  activities  during  the  final  year  and 
to  decide  the  program's  future  when  Institute  support  ended. 


A  PARENT  SPEAKS 

Following  is  a  slightly  abridged  version  of  an  interview  with  a  mother 
who  had  two  daughters  in  one  of  the  cross-cultural  nursery  schools.  One 
girl  attended  the  full  three  years  of  this  NIMH-supported  project.  The 
other  was  enrolled  during  the  final  two  years  and  now  attends  the  project's 
independently-financed  successor,  in  which  the  mother  is  a  teacher's  aide. 

Mother:  Everytime  I  talk  about  it,  I  get  sort  of  shook  up  inside  because 
they  learned  so  much,  and  I  have  learned  so  much.  I  don't  mean  only 
about  toys  and  things  like  that  but  the  most  important-that  I  have 
learned  how  to  raise  children.  Because  I  sort  of  talk  and  raise  my  girls 
differently  from  what  I  did  my  older  boys.  And  because  1  didn't  know  -. 

Here  is  an  instance.  I  was  a  very  over-protective  mother  and  I'm  a  very 
firm  mother.  Well,  in  the  NICE  project  I  was  taught  how  to  help  the 
situation.  I  used  to  spank  a  lot,  you  know.  But  I  learned  you  can  talk  to 
children-sit  down  and  talk  to  them  just  like  you  and  I  talk. 

Like  when  we  first  went  to  the  nursery  school.  When  the  children  was 
all  2  and  2  and  1/2,  they  would  hit  each  other  a  lot-and  the  teacher  would 


The  NICE  schools  closed  in  1969,  after  three  years,  and  the  children 
went  off  to  kindergarten  in  public  schools.  Thanks  to  decisions  by  the 
Parent  Advisoiy  Council  and  the  commitment  of  Mary  Lane  and  her 
associates,  however,  the  end  of  NICE  was  the  beginning  of  CCFC,  which 
stands  for  Cross-Cultural  Family  Center.  This  is  an  organization  with  the 
same  goal  as  the  original  program— the  promotion  of  interfamily,  mtei- 
race  relations— and  a  membership  comprising  33  of  the  families  who  were 
in  NICE  and  a  number  of  new  ones.  It  operates  a  nursery  school  taught 
largely  by  mothers  who  were  tiained  on  the  job  in  the  original  cross- 
cultural  schools,  and  a  kindergarten-supplement  program  for  the  original 
NICE  children.  It  also  offers  afternoon  and  evening  programs  for  other 
children  and  for  parents.  The  Center  depends  on  fund-raising  activities  by 
its  members  and  on  tuition  fees  from  the  children  of  nonmembers.  It  is 
pleasantly  housed  in  the  new  church  school  building  of  the  Unitarian 
Church,  in  the  area  served  by  the  original  project. 

The  accomplishments  of  NICE  may  be  summarized  as  follows; 

1 .  The  children  at  5  seemed  to  be  without  racial  or  class  prejudice.  As  a 
matter  of  fact,  on  the  basis  of  observations  by  project  and  teaching  staffs, 
the  children  got  along  throughout  the  project  at  least  as  well  as  any 
homogenous  group  of  children.  Friendships  across  group  lines  were  com- 
mon. During  the  second  year,  teachers  reported  31  "best  friend"  pairs.  In 
more  than  half  the  cases,  these  were  inter-racial.  Since  the  children  were 
2  years  old  when  the  project  began,  they  have  little  or  no  conscious 
memory  of  associations  in  groups  of  children  before  that  time.  They  will 
remember  only  playing  with  children  of  cross-cultural  backgrounds. 

2.  The  children  made  gains  in  intellectual  development,  as  shown  in 
Table  1 .  On  the  Stanford-Binet  intelligence  scale  the  average  I.Q.  gain  over 
three  years  was  nine  points-from  102  to  111.  On  the  Peabody  Picture 
Vocabulary  Test,  which  tests  a  child's  understanding  of  words,  it  was  24 
points-from  90  to  114.  The  gains  in  all  cases  were  significant  at  the  .01 
level,  and  there  were  no  significant  differences  between  housing  groups. 


walk  up  and  say:  "She  doesn't  want  you  to  hit  her.  It  hurts  when  you 
hit."  And  I'm  just  looking  and  observing.  And  then  when  the  child  would 
hit  again,  the  teacher  would  say:  "No,  that  hurts,  and  she  doesn't  want 
you  to  hit  and  we're  not  going  to  let  you  hit."  It  was  so  fascinating  to  me, 
I  really  wanted  to  stay  every  day  just  to  watch  how  they  handled  the 
children. 

You  would  have  acted  differently? 

Right,  I  would  have  wanted  to  say:  "Now  listen,  you  know  better  than 
to  hit,"  and  I  would  have  said,  "Now  you  hit  her  back!"  But  that's  not  the 
way,  I've  learned.  There  was  my  little  girl  and  another  little  girl,  which  is  a 
little  Japanese,  Neisha.  Every  day  this  little  girl,  sometime  during  the  day 
she  would  walk  over  and  just  hit  my  little  girl.  And  my  little  girl  would 
start  crying.  You  know  me-I'd  say,  "This  is  just  killing  ray  child!"  So  one 
day  I  got  so  mad  about  it  I  went  to  the  teacher  and  I  told  her,  "Now 
listen,  I'm  just  sick  and  tired  of  my  child  getting  beat  up  every  day,"  and  I 
said,  "Something  should  be  done  about  it."  And  the  teacher,  very  calmly: 
"Well,  have  you  any  idea  why  Neisha  hits  her  all  the  time?"  And  I  said  I'd 


White  children  as  a  group  went  from  108  on  the  Binet  test  to  121 ;  black 
children,  from  97  to  103.  On  the  Peabody,  white  children  went  from  93 
to  117;  black,  from  83  to  110.  These  differences  between  racial  groups 
were  significant  at  the  .0 1  level ,  with  the  white  children  scoring  higher  than 
the  black  children  at  all  time-points.  The  staff  points  out,  however,  that  it 
is  impossible  to  group  the  children  on  the  basis  of  a  single  factor,  either 
race  or  type  of  residence,  without  the  other  factor  entering  in,  Oriental 
children  were  omitted  from  analyses  involving  racial  groups  because  there 
were  too  few  for  proper  statistical  treatment..' 

TABLE  1 
Average  IQ  Scores  by  Type  of  Residence,  First  and  Last  Tests 

Public  Random          St.  Francis 

Ho  using  ^          Housing^  Square^ 

First      Last      First      Last      First      Last 

Stanford-Binet  98        103       100       113        108       117 

Peabody  83        115         92       108         94       117 

1.  Low  income;  black. 

2.  All  income  levels;  about  half  black,  half  white  and  mixed. 

3.  Middle  income;  about  2/3  white,  1/3  black. 

3.  The  children  made  significant  gains  in  social  competence  as 
measured  on  the  California  Preschool  Social  Competency  Scale.  This  scale 
covers  a  wide  range  of  behaviors  such  as  response  to  routine,  response  to 
the  unfamiliar,  following  instructions,  making  explanations,  helping 
others,  initiating  activities,  reacting  to  frustration,  and  accepting  limits. 


not  even  thought  about  why  she  was  hitting  her.  And  the  teacher  said, 
"She  wants  to  be  loved."  The  teacher  went  on  to  tell  me.  "When  they  are 
that  little,  they  don't  know  how  to  talk;  this  is  their  way  of  com- 
municating with  one  another.  That's  it.  And  the  only  thing  she  is  trying  to 
tell  Mary  is1  'I  want  to  play  with  you.  I  want  to  be  your  friend.' "  And 
within  a  month  and  a  half  those  two  were  playing  together  every  day.  But 
you  know,  my  concept  of  it  was  a  completely  different  thing,  but  the 
wrong  thing. 

I  wish  the  project  had  come  around  sooner,  that  my  older  children 
wouldn't  have  had  to  suffer  so  much  because  I  din't  know.  And  I  think 
that  the  parents  in  this  project  got  out  of  it  just  as  much  as  these  children 
or  even  more. 

My  oldest  girl  was  in  the  nursery  school  for  three  years.  Well,  I'm  just 
so  proud  of  her  in  the  kindergarten.  She's  marvelous  in  her  class,  and  they 
are  getting  her  ready  to  read.  Because  all  the  stuff  that  the  older  children 
have  done,  she's  had  it.  You  know,  too,  at  times  she  helps  the  teacher  a 
lot-to  help  the  other  children.  And  so  I'm  very  proud  of  my  children, 
those  who  were  in  the  project.  Because  they  have  learned  education-wise 
and  they  have  learned  one  of  the  things  -. 


The  national  norms  for  this  scale  are  based  on  teacher  ratings  of  children 
in  preschool  programs.  On  the  first  test,  the  average  score  placed  the 
NICE  children  at  the  38th  percentile;  on  the  last  test,  at  the  78th.  The 
gains  were  significant  for  each  housing  group,  and  the  differences  among 
groups  were  also  significant.  Over  the  thiee-yeai  period,  there  was  no 
significant  diffeience  between  white  children,  who  went  from  the  41st 
percentile  to  the  77th,  and  black  children,  who  went  from  the  35th 
percentile  to  the  76th. 

TABLE  2 
California  Preschool  Social  Competency  Scale  (Percentiles) 

First  Test  Last  Test 

Public  Housing1  31  72 

Random  Housing^  43  82 

St.  Francis  Square3  42  78 

1.  Low  income;  black. 

2.  All  income  levels;  about  half  black,  half  white  and  mixed. 

3.  Middle  income;  about  2/3  white,  1/3  black. 


4.  The  children  made  significant  gains,  too,  on  three  scales  developed 
by  the  project  in  an  effort  to  guage  three  qualities  it  was  trying  to  instill— 
basic  trust,  initiative,  and  autonomy.  Among  the  housing  groups  no 
significant  difference  appeared  except  in  autonomy,  where  the  St.  Francis 
Square  children  rated  higher  than  the  other  groups  at  all  times.  When  first 
tested,  white  children  scored  significantly  higher  than  black  in  trust  and 
initiative  but  not  on  autonomy;  on  the  last  test,  the  scores  for  whites  and 


Just  like  my  older  daughter,  she  can  tell  the  difference  between  a 
Japanese  and  a  Chinese.  And  I  can't.  Because  she  has  been  witli  these  little 
Japanese  and  Chinese  children.  I'll  say;  "Oh,  that's  a  Japanese,"  and  she'd 
say,  "Oh,  Mamma,  that's  not  a  Japanese-that's  a  Chinese!"  And  this  really 
shocked  me  because  I  couldn't  tell  the  difference.  But  through  us  having 
all  the  different  races  Of  the  children,  and  them  playing  with  them  -. 
Sometime  you'll  kind  of  look  at  a  child  and  they'll  be  looking  at  a  child 
and  looking  at  the  child-they're  really  looking  at  the  child,  different  from 
the  way  we  look  at  it.  They  look  at  how  the  eyes  are  made,  and  how  the 
nose,  and  how  everything,  you  know. 

Anybody  tell  you  that? 

No—but  I  sort  of  learned  from  my  children.  Children  look  at  each  other 
in  a  different  way  than  we  look  at  each  other.  And  then,  you  know,  they'll 
go  up  and  touch,  you  know,  to  make  sure  that  the  hair- they  want  to 
know  why  is  her  hair  different  from  mine.  And  they  have  to  touch  it  to 
really  feel  and  see  that  that  is  different  and  everything.  So  kids  is  just 
amazing.  And  when  I  started  in  the  nursery  I  would  go  almost  just  for 


black  on  each  of  the  characteristics  were  virtually  identical,  The  three 
scales  are  experimental  and  their  validity  untested. 

5.  In  their  attitudes  toward  members  of  other  racial  and  socioeconomic 
groups,  the  parents  have  become  considerably  more  open-minded  and 
accepting.  This  is  the  impression  of  the  staff  and  of  persons  who  talk  to 
representative  parents.  It  is  substantiated  by  an  analysis  of  ratings  for  each 
mother  on  several  experimental  scales  measuring  "intergroup  accept- 
ance." The  ratings  were  made  at  the  start  of  the  project  and  again  at  the 
end.  Over  the  three  years,  both  the  average  score  of  all  the  mothers  and 
the  average  score  of  the  mothers  in  each  housing  group  increased  sig- 
nificantly. The  groups  had  the  same  relative  positions  at  the  end  as  at  the 
beginning;   first,  St.   Francis  Square;  second,  Random  Housing;  third, 
Public  Housing. 

6.  The   mothers   have   become   more   skilled    as    parents  and   more 
competent  as  members  of  society.  Ratings  on  experimental  scales  to 
measure  child-rearing  practices  and  general  social  competence  showed 
significant  increases  over  the  three  years.  The  St.  Francis  Square  group 
again  rated  first  and  the  public  housing  group  third.  Four  of  the  mothers 
have  become  assistant  teachers  in  the  CCFC  nursery;  another  is  in  charge 
of  the  Center's  kindergarten-supplement  program;  another  supervises  the 
after-school  activities  for  older  children  at  the  Center.  One  mother  has 
been  employed  by  the  San  Francisco  Unified  School  District  as  a  school 
aide.  A  mother  who  was  on  welfare  when  the  pioject  started  has  become  a 
secretary  to  a  Y.W.C.A. 

7.  Through  open  meetings,  social  affaiis,  membership  of  parents  in 
community  organizations,  and  other  ways,  the  project's  influence  has 
extended  beyond  the  families  immediately  involved.  Staff  and  parents 
have  been  called  on  to  consult  Head  Start  staff,  participate  in  panels, 
appear  on  TV,  and  help  the  State  prepare  for  the  1970  White  House 
Conference  on  Children  and  Youth.  The  NICE  schools  were  used  in  the 
training  of  psychiatric  social  workers,  teachers,  home  economics  students, 

observation  and  then  the  more  I  went,  then  the  more  I  wanted  to  go.  I 
wanted  to  learn.  Then  I  started  working  in  the  school  with  some  of  the 
children,  which  was  very  rewarding,  because  you  teach  them  a  lot,  and  you 
learn  a  lot  from  them. 

Has  it  been  easier  in  kindergarten  for  your  girl  than  for  your  boys? 

It's  a  lot  easier.  It's  so  much  different  in  boys  and  girls  anyway;  girls 
seem  to  be  a  little  bit  more  advanced  than  boys  when  it  comes  to  learning. 
But  my  daughter,  she  is  as  much  different  in  her  going  to  the  kindergarten 
than  when  my  sons  went  as  day  and  night.  Because  when  she  went  in  there 
she  didn't  have  this  fear  of  being  away  from  me,  for  a  time,  because  she 
had  learned  that,  and  then  she  had  learned  how  to  play  with  other  children 
and  she  had  learned  all  these  different  kind  of  peoples.  Just  like  a  teacher 
m  kindergarten  is  a  Japanese  and  they  have  an  aide  in  there  that's  a  Negro. 
Well,  she  goes  in  and  she  doesn't  even  think  about  race  or  why  is  a  person 
different. 

She  notices  that  -? 

In  the  Nice  project,  even  when  they  was  two  and  a  half  and  three  they 
noticed.  And  they  wondered  and  they  questioned  the  teachers  to  some 


health  personnel,  and  members  of  the  Teachers  Corps  and  the  Neighbor- 
hood Youth  Corps. 

8.  Materials  useful  to  other  projects  concerned  with  preschool  children 
have  been  developed.  These  include  descriptions  of  the  home  task  pro- 
gram, which  has  been  widely  followed  by  Head  Start  and  other  projects 
for  disadvantaged  children,  and  of  suggested  processes  and  materials  for  a 
"multicultural  curriculum,"  and  a  film,  "Swimming  in  the  Nursery 
School."  Two  films  are  being  developed  from  NICE  video  tapes-on  the 
introduction  of  a  child  into  an  ongoing  progiam  and  on  the  use  of 
cognitive  materials  to  stimulate  thinking.  A  series  of  pamphlets  on  various 
aspects  of  the  program  is  planned. 

If  money  becomes  available,  the  team  that  directed  NICE— Mary  Lane 
and  two  research  associates,  Mary  S.  Lewis  and  Freeman  F.  Elzey-expect 
to  follow  the  NICE  children  through  the  fifth  grade  of  public  school. 


Birth  of  NICE 

One  day  eight  years  ago,  Mary  Lane  received  a  telephone  call  from  an 
acquaintance  of  hers,  a  young  mother  who  had  recently  moved  into  St. 
Francis  Square.  This  is  the  cooperative  apartment  development,  for 
families  of  middle  income,  that  in  1962  replaced  several  blocks  of  old, 
three-story  houses  occupied  mainly  by  low-income  black  families.  Other 
blocks  of  similar  houses  had  been  torn  down  earlier  to  make  room  for 
luxurious  high-rise  apartments.  The  San  Francisco  State  professor,  an 
authority  on  the  education  of  young  children,  was  asked  to  talk  with  a 
group  of  "the  Square"  parents  about  the  troubling  experiences  they  were 
having. 

Dr.  Lane  knew  that  the  development  had  been  planned  as  an  integrated 
community  for  people  committed  to  inter-racial  living  and  that,  while  the 
majority  of  the  residents  were  white,  a  number  of  middle-income  black 

length  about  why,  you  know.  You  know,  just  like  the  car  toon -they 
would  rub  the  black  children's  skin;  they  would  just  nib  it  to  see  would  it 
come  off.  And  then  they  ask  a  lot  of  questions.  Just  like  m  my  daughter, 
she  wanted  her  hair  to  be  long  like  Enrica's  hair,  and  so  I  had  to  press  hers 
out  so  hers  would  be  long  like  'Rica's  hair. 

Do  they  ask  questions  of  you? 

Yes,  Because  they  are  interested  in  things.  And  there  she  was,  sort  of 
questionning.  She'd  say:  "Am  I  really  a  black  person  or  am  I  a  blue 
person?"  The  way  I  feel  about  it  she  is  a  Negro,  but  sometimes  other 
people  call  the  Negro  a  black.  And  then  she  said:  "Well,  I  think  I  want  to 
be  a  Negro."  I  felt  kind  of  fine.  You  know.  And  after  the  questions,  it 
wasn't  no  problem  at  all.  They  played  with  each  other.  They'd  even  come 
and  they'd  say:  I'm  a  Negro,  or  black,"  and  "You're  a  Caucasian,"  and 
"You're  a  Japanese,"  and  "You're  so  and  so." 

How  did  the  teachers  explain? 

Well,  they'd  say  at  times:  "Some  of  us  are  one  way  and  some  of  us  the 
other,  you  know,  but  we're  all  human  beings  because  we  are  all  boys  and 

girls." 


families  lived  there,  too.  She  now  learned  that  many,  probably  most,  of 
the  parents  in  the  Square  felt  frustrated  and  rebuffed.  They  had  moved  in 
considering  themselves  liberals  in  racial  matters  and  eager  to  get  along 
with  the  black  people  in  the  surrounding  neighborhood,  mainly  in  public 
housing.  But  facilities  in  the  development  such  as  laundry  rooms  and 
playgrounds  were  being  vandalized,  and  children  from  the  Square  were 
being  attacked  and  chased  home  fiom  school  by  public  housing  children. 
Further,  a  group  of  parents  had  started  a  cooperative  nursery  school  in  a 
nearby  Y.W.C.A.  and  had  hoped  that  some  of  the  black  public  housing 
residents  would  participate,  thus  giving  children  from  the  Square  an 
opportunity  to  associate  with  other  children  who  would  be  entering 
kindergarten  or  first  grade  with  them.  The  public  housing  people  had  not 
responded.  Residents  of  the  Square  were  beginning  to  feel  themselves  on 
an  island  surrounded  by  hostility. 

The  Square  children,  Dr.  Lane  was  told,  were  doing  very  well  in 
elementary  school,  where  almost  all  of  the  pupils  were  black.  The  tcacheis 
liked  the  newcomers  and  were  pushing  them  ahead.  "And,"  one  mother 
reported,  "27  of  us  have  been  appointed  room  mothers!"  Apparently  it  had 
not  occurred  to  her  to  wonder  about  the  women  who  had  been  the  room 
mothers  before.  And  apparently  it  had  not  occurred  to  most  of  the 
parents  to  wonder  how  public  housing  people  might  feel  when  they 
looked  out  of  their  stark  concrete  dwelling  place  and  saw  the  Square- 
with  its  attractive  buildings  and  the  neighborhood's  only  grass  and 
shrubbery-occupying  the  site  where  they  or  relatives  or  friends  once  had 
lived. 

Says  Dr.  Lane,  "I  thought  to  myself:  Here  are  all  these  middle-income 
children- 200  preschoolers  alone-in  the  Square,  And  here  are  all  these 
children  in  public  housing.  How  wonderful  if  we  could  get  a  cross-cultural 
thing  going,  and  see  if  we  could  break  down  some  of  the  barriers.  " 

A  family-centered  nursery  school  for  2-year-olds  would  be  an  ideal 
"cross-cultural  thing,"  she  felt,  because  even  people  of  the  most  diverse 


I  was  prejudiced  before  I  went  to  the  nursery  school.  Because  when  my 
older  sons  would  come  home  and  would  talk  about  then  friends,  I  would 
say;  Is  he  a  Negro  or  white?"  And  one  day  my  oldest  asked  me:  "Well 
Mamma  what  difference  does  it  make?"  And  that  gave  me  something  to 
think  about  And  then  I  said:  "Well,  really  what  difference  docs  it  really 
make?"  And  then  1  stopped  asking  him  that.  X 

This  NICE  project  has  helped  me  to  overcome  my  prejudice,  because 
we  often  have  discussions  and  one  discussion  was  on  p  e  ud  ce  and  1  S 
hr  e  or  four  sessions  were  on  ,t.  And  at  the  first  meeting,  everybody  was 
sort  of,  you  know,  not  really  saying  what  they  really  feel  So  Mrs  L7o 
she  was  there.  "Now,"  she  says,  "You  should  be  fair-you  willy  feel  one 
way  or  the  other  about  it."  And  by  the  time  we  got  to  the  econd  nVeeting 
.telly  came  out,  what  you  really  feel.  And  then  we  learned  about  each 


10 


interests,  if  properly  approached,  would  surely  work  together  more  or  less 
harmoniously  for  the  welfare  of  their  children.  And  working  together, 
they  would  learn  to  understand  and  appreciate  one  another.  Their 
children  would  accept  inter-racial  activities  as  a  matter  of  couise.  Ad- 
ditionally, the  children  fiom  disadvantaged  families  piobably  would  enter 
the  public  school  system,  after  three  years  in  the  nursery  school,  much 
better  prepared  intellectually  and  emotionally  than  could  be  expected 
otherwise. 

With  the  moral  support  of  a  number  of  community  organizations  and 
financial  backing  from  NIMH,  through  a  grant  to  the  College,  Dr.  Lane 
spent  a  year  developing  her  ideas,  finding  sites  for  the  schools,  talking  to 
families  with  children  of  the  right  age  (only  two  such  families  declined  to 
enter  their  children),  and  selecting  and  training  the  teaching  staff.  Assist- 
ing her  through  the  life  of  the  project  were  the  research  associates 
mentioned  earlier,  Freeman  Elzey  and  Mary  Lewis.  The  three  worked 
together  as  a  team,  Elzey  designing  the  research  projects  and  analyzing 
results,  Mrs.  Lewis  conducting  the  interviews  and  creating  the  home  tasks 
program,  and  all  three  sharing  administrative  duties.  A  psychiatric  social 
worker,  Stanley  Seidertnan  Joined  the  staff  some  months  after  the  schools 
opened  and  became  a  part  of  the  team  in  a  counselor  role. 


Key  Philosophy;  Complete  Acceptance 

Members  of  the  teaching  staff  faced  more  problems  than  usual  because, 
in  addition  to  being  teachers,  they  were  expected  to  act  as  counselors  to 
parents  and  researchers,  a  function  that  obliged  them  to  make  daily  notes 
and  write  a  weekly  record  of  both  child  and  parental  behavior.  During 
much  of  the  first  year,  too,  they  sometimes  found  themselves  acting  as 
social  workers,  guiding  low-income  families  to  medical,  housing,  and  other 
community  services.  Even  after  a  social  worker  was  added  to  the  project 


Are  you  from  San  Francisco? 

No,  I'm  from  the  South  Houston.  And  I  remember  one  of  the  things 
that  used  to  hurt  me— when  my  mother  would  say  Yes,  ma'm  and  No'm  to 
a  12-year-old  Caucasian.  That  used  to  break  my  heart,  because  that  isn't 
right,  you  know,  ffow'rp  you  .  .  .  How 're  you ....  And  as  a  child  coming 
up,  it  would  break  my  heart,  because  we  were  taught  at  home  that  you 
respect  age.  And  you  know,  I  could  understand  that.  But  a  person  youngei 
than  me!  And  I  would  ask  my  mother,  but  she  never  could  really  explain 
to  me,  to  my  satisfaction.  Or  she'd  say:  "Well,  you  have  to,"  or  "That's 
the  rules."  And  I  have  always  questioned  things  like  that, 

There  were  things  that  came  out  during  the  group  discussions  here,  that 
one  race  don't  really  know  what  the  other  one  really  thinks  until  you 
really  get  down  and  start  talking  about  things.  Because  just  like  some  of 
the  white  persons  would  say  they  were  taught  you  shouldn't  marry  out  of 
your  race  and  everything,  and  a  whole  lot  of  this  your  paren  ts  don 't  wan  t 
you  to.  And  they  were  shocked  to  know  that  these  Negro  parents  didn't 
want  their  children  to  marry  out  of  their  race.  You  know,  and  different 
things  like  this. 

11 


staff,  some  of  the  families  preferred  to  take  their  troubles  to  the  teachers. 
A  summer  of  intensive  training  preceded  the  opening  of  the  schools  but 
could  not  altogether  prepare  the  teaching  staff  for  what  lay  ahead. 

Probably  the  most  important  feature  differentiating  these  cross-cultural 
schools  from  other  nursery  schools  was  unconditional  acceptance  of  the 
parents  as  well  as  the  children.  It  was  also  the  teachers'  heaviest  burden, 
Unconditional  acceptance  meant,  for  one  thing,  putting  up  cheerfully 
with  lackadaisical  observance  of  schedule.  Children  and  their  mothers 
were  welcomed  any  time  they  arrived.  Mothers  who  wanted  to  take 
children  home  before  the  session  was  over  were  permitted  to  do  so. 
Mothers  who  failed  to  pick  up  their  children  on  time  found  them  being 
cared  for. 

More  disturbing  practices  also  were  accepted.  One  mother  spanked  her 
2-year-old  daughter  for  wetting  her  pants.  One  father,  finding  his  boy 
reluctant  to  leave  at  the  end  of  a  session,  took  off  his  belt  and  threatened 
him.  A  woman,  watching  her  son,  not  yet  3,  color  a  picture,  insisted  that 
he  stay  within  the  lines.  In  each  case  the  teachers  felt  shivers  running  up 
and  down  their  spines  but  said  nothing. 

In  staff  meetings,  where  problems  associated  with  unconditional  accept- 
ance were  often  raised,  Mary  Lane  and  her  team  took  the  position  that  if 
parents  were  ever  to  be  open  to  change,  in  either  child-rearing  practices  01 
racial  attitudes,  they  first  had  to  be  accepted  as  they  were.  Some  staff 
members  found  it  harder  than  others  to  go  along.  One  head  teacher  left 
after  a  few  months. 

Before  the  end  of  the  first  year,  the  staff  had  evidence  that  the  concept 
of  complete  acceptance  was  paying  off.  Families  were  changing  their  time 
habits  to  conform  with  the  schedule  of  the  schools.  Parents  were  asking 
teachers  why  they  didn't  punish  the  children  for  wetting  themselves, 
threaten  them  for  not  doing  immediately  what  the  teacher  wanted,  insist 
on  neat  performances.  And  the  parents  were  beginning  to  listen  to  the 
answers. 


Making  the  Parents  Welcome  and  Useful 

When  the  three  schools  opened,  parents  were  invited  to  stay  with  their 
children  as  long  as  they  wished  or  felt  it  necessary.  Each  school  had  a 
parents'  corner,  fitted  out  with  comfortable  chairs,  coffee-making 

I  owe  a  lot  to  this  project.  I  had  a  distrust  for  professional  people,  you 
know-that  they  were  one  way,  and  you  were  another  way.  But  then  in 
this  project  I  learned  that  although  they  are  professional  people  they  are 
just  human  beings,  like  I  am.  You  know,  we  call  the  staff  by  their  first 
names,  and  everybody  always  looks  at  me  when  I  say  Mrs.  Lane.  Most  of 
us  call  her  "Mary,"  but  I  don't  know-I  have  so  much  respect  for  her.  I  just 
come  from  old-fashioned  teaching  that  when  you  really  respect  a  person, 
and  admire  a  person-.  When  I  say,  "Mrs.  Lane,"  well,  this  is  my  way  of 
saying  1  respect  her  as  a  person  for  what  she  stands  for  and  the  things  that 
she  has  done.  It's  not  because  there  is  a  difference  in  race  or  anything  like 
that;  it  just  comes  out  of  respect.  I  suppose  there  is  something  in  me  that  I 
just  can't  come  to  call  her  "Mary."  I  just  can't. 
12 


materials,  and  magazines.  Some  of  the  mothers,  delighted  at  the  op- 
portunity to  watch  their  children  and  to  talk  with  other  adults,  lingered 
half  the  morning.  After  a  while  the  "bag  lunch"  became  an  institution.  On 
a  certain  day  each  week,  mothers  would  drop  in  with  a  lunch  bag  during 
the  morning  and  stay  to  observe  the  rest  of  the  session  and  to  eat  with  the 
staff.  These  informal  get-togethers  gradually  moved  from  general  talk 
about  children  to  guided  discussions  of  such  subjects  as  sibling  rivalry,  art 
activities  in  the  home,  discipline,  and  toy  selection. 

Mothers  became  more  and  more  deeply  involved  in  school  operations. 
After  a  few  months,  some  of  them  were  asked  to  volunteer  as  additional 
aides  because,  even  with  one  or  two  student  teachers  in  each  school,  the 
ratio  of  adults  to  2-year-olds  was  not  high  enough  to  give  the  children  as 
much  individual  attention  as  they  needed.  Later,  with  the  approach  of 
summer  and  the  departure  of  the  student  teachers,  all  mothers  were  asked 
to  serve  one  day  a  week  during  the  summer  session.  Most  of  those  whose 
time  was  not  otherwise  committed  agreed.  They  were  invited  to  bring 
along  their  children,  so  the  workday  became  also  a  family  outing  day. 
The  following  fall,  Dr.  Lane  explained  at  a  parents'  meeting  that  writing 
research  reports  and  attending  teachers'  meetings  required  a  sizeable 
amount  of  the  teaching  staffs'  time.  So  one  day  a  week  the  schools  would 
close  unless  the  parents  could  take  over.  Teams  of  mothers  signed  up  to 
do  this,  each  team  obligating  itself  for  one  day  a  week  foi  six  weeks. 

Home  Tasks 

At  the  end  of  the  first  year,  project  staff  members  and  teachers 
wondered  what  else  could  be  done  to  bring  home  and  school  together.  In 
about  half  the  families,  parents  rarely  visited  the  schools  except  to  drop 
off  or  pick  up  the  child.  And  some  of  the  parents  who  did  stay  longer 
were  either  uninterested  in  or  puzzled  by  what  was  going  on,  in  spite  of 
staff  efforts  to  communicate.  These  were  parents  who  in  the  main  thought 
of  learning  only  in  terms  of  reading  and  writing.  Some  way  of  getting  into 
the  homes  and  talking  to  the  mothers  individually  seemed  necessary  if 
NICE  was  to  be  sure  it  was  reaching  at  least  most  of  the  families.  The  staff 
decided  on  an  educational  intervention  scheme  it  called  the  Home  Task 
Project. 

Home  Tasks  would  help  parents  see  and  make  use  of  the  learning 
potential  in  the  play  activities  of  their  young  children.  Parents  then  would 


1  would  just  like  to  see  that  this  family  center  could  be  continued, 
because  I  think  it  has  helped  my  children  and  so  many  of  the  children  that 
need  help.  And  not  only  the  children,  but  the  parents.  There  was  one 
mother  in  particular.  Her  children  were  enrolled  in  the  nursery  school,  and 
the  first  two  years  she  wasn't  really  concerned  about  taking  care  of  them. 
You  know-their  eating  right  and  things  like  that.  And  since  working  with 
the  NICE  project  and  the  teachers  in  it,  I  have  seen  a  big  change  in  her. 
She's  a  completely  different  person.  And  then  you  ponder.  You  see  a 
parent  that  don't  care,  and  after  three  years,  this  parent  has  just  turned 
about.  It's  the  sort  of  thing  that  makes  you  stop  and  think. 

13 


have  greater  undeistanding  of  the  school's  task  and  of  the  way  the  school 
uses  materials  to  help  the  child  learn.  Also,  by  working  with  the  child  a 
few  minutes  each  day,  the  mother  would  begin  to  view  heiself  as  the 
child's  teacher  as  well  as  his  mother. 

Mary  Lewis  spent  the  summer  developing  the  home  tasks  and  the 
explanatory  materials  to  accompany  them.  The  tasks  were  designed  to 
extend  knowledge,  sharpen  peiception,  develop  motor  skills,  or  expand 
concepts.  They  included  seeds  to  be  planted,  a  scrapbook  to  be  filled,  a 
plank  to  be  balanced  on,  picture  books  to  be  looked  at,  puzzles  to  be 
worked,  pieces  of  wood  to  be  joined,  drawings  to  be  colored,  games  to  be 
played,  and  a  turtle  to  be  cared  for.  At  Halloween  every  family  would  get 
a  pumpkin,  along  with  a  recipe  for  pumpkin  cookies,  a  candle,  and  a  list  of 
suggestions  on  how  a  pumpkin  could  be  used  to  help  the  child  grasp  such 
concepts  as  round,  orange,  hard,  and  hollow.  Altogether,  60  such  tasks 
were  developed. 

Every  week,  at  a  regular,  mutually  satisfactory  time,  each  home  was 
visited  by  a  member  of  the  staff  or  a  student  teacher  fringing  the  week's 
task  and  typewritten  instructions  for  its  use.  The  visitor  demonstrated  the 
task,  explained  the  instructions,  and  discussed  the  educational  value.  A 
bean  bag  game,  for  example,  was  intended  to  develop  not  only  eye-hand 
coordination  but  also  such  space-relation  concepts  as  inside,  outside,  to 
the  right,  to  the  left,  over,  and  in  front  of.  A  "feel  kit"  included  the  book, 
"What  Is  Your  Favorite  Tiling  to  Touch?",  a  bag  for  holding  small  objects 
to  be  identified  by  touch,  and  instructions  for  playing  the  "comparison 
game,"  in  which  the  child  is  encouraged  to  name  things  that  are  "as 

smooth  as  — ,"  "as  sticky  as  — ,"  "as  soft  as ,"  and  so  on.  One  task 

called  for  mother  and  child  to  take  a  short  walk,  talk  about  what  they  saw, 
and  draw  a  rough  map  locating  some  of  the  things  seen.  Mothers  were 
encouraged  to  use  the  tasks  with  their  children  at  least  once  a  day.  The 
following  week  the  visitor  returned  with  the  next  task  and  asked  about 
the  child's  experiences  with  the  last  one  and  the  mother's  ideas  for 
expanding  its  usefulness  as  an  educational  item.  Expensive  items  such  as 
books  or  puzzles  were  picked  up  foi  delivery  to  other  families. 

The  staff  reports  that  the  home  task  project,  which  continued  through 
the  second  year  and  18  weeks  of  the  third,  was  not  without  headaches. 
Items  were  lost,  broken,  or  kept  for  longer  periods  than  scheduled,  and 
visits  were  missed  because  of  forgetfulness  either  by  the  visitor  or  the 
family.  From  the  comments  of  the  mothers,  however,  the  staff  believes 
that  the  project  succeeded  both  in  drawing  parents  closer  to  the  schools 
and  in  demonstrating  the  potential  of  the  parents  as  teachers.  Visitors 
reported  increasing  interest  on  the  part  of  most  families.  Many  mothers 
reported  that  home  task  day  had  become  a  special  occasion  for  the  whole 
family.  Even  middle-class  mothers  were  surprised  at  the  variety  of  ways 
open  to  them  to  develop  their  children's  abilities.  After  each  visit  certain 
information  was  recorded  on  IBM  cards.  It  is  now  possible  to  determine, 
among  other  things,  which  tasks  were  most  used  and  how  a  given  task  was 
regarded  by  mothers  and  children. 

14 


A  Training  Course  for  Parents 

At  the  request  of  mothers  who  said  they  would  like  more  information 
on  how  to  work  with  young  children,  the  project  staff  decided  midway 
through  the  second  year  to  offer  a  course  in  preschool  learning  and 
education.  It  reasoned  that  such  a  course  would  make  the  women  both 
better  mothers  and  more  competent  aides  at  the  schools.  Mothers  were 
told  the  course  would  lun  two  hours  a  week  for  15  weeks  and  that  they 
would  be  expected  to  work  one  day  a  week  in  the  nurseiy.  Out  of  32 
mothers  who  were  not  working,  15  signed  up— 8  of  them  black,  4  white, 
and  3  Oriental. 

Asked  by  Dr.  Lane,  the  instructor,  why  they  wanted  training,  those 
who  signed  up  said  they  would  like  to  learn,  among  other  things: 

To  talk  to  my  children  so  they  will  want  to  mind. 

To  get  rid  of  the  feeling  that  I  can't  handle  my  child. 

To  read  books  with  more  feeling. 

To  stay  calm. 

Educational  spots  in  the  city  for  children. 

What  to  expect  of  children. 

To  understand  myself  better. 

The  mothers  were  asked  to  take  notes;  readings  were  assigned  and 
reported  on;  small  groups  were  formed  to  make  a  special  study  of  one  area 
of  interest  chosen  by  the  members  (such  as  books,  trips,  toys);  and  each 
mother  had  a  semester  project-to  study  and  report  upon  one  child,  not 
her  own. 

To  set  the  tone  for  the  course,  Dr.  Lane  wrote  each  mother  a  letter  in 
which  she  said,  in  part: 

You  are  your  child's  most  important  teacher.  Did  you  ever  stop  to 
think  what  life-long  learnings  come  from  you?  First  of  all,  he  learns  the 
feel  of  mother  as  you  feed  him,  change  him,  bathe  him.  He  remembeis 
this  always.  From  you  he  learns  how  much  he  can  trust  the  world. 

Then  he  learns  his  language  from  you.  His  voice  tone  and  the  way  he 
pronounces  words  will  be  much  like  yours.  What  he  first  talks  about 
will  be  what  he  has  heard  and  seen  at  your  knee. 

Very  important  is  what  he  learns  from  you  about  how  you  feel  about 
people.  If  you  are  friendly  and  helpful  and  think  people  are  pretty  fine, 
he  is  likely  to  feel  this  way,  too. 

He  learns  very  early  from  you  how  you  feel  about  him.  If  you  feel  your 
child  is  just  great  for  a  two-  or  three-year-old,  he'll  feel  great  about 
himself.  These  attitudes  that  he  "catches"  from  you  when  a  child,  he  is 
likely  to  keep  for  all  his  life.  . .  . 

The  course  included  sessions  on  child  study,  art  activities,  music  and 
fantasy,  books  and  story-telling,  educational  trips  and  toys,  and  science, 
Dr.  Lane  or  another  San  Francisco  State  faculty  member  would  discuss 

15 


the  topic  and  answer  questions.  Then  a  few  of  the  weekly  reports  written 
by  the  mothers  as  part  of  their  semester  project-to  observe  a 
child—would  be  read  and  discussed.  During  the  following  week  the 
mothers  were  expected  to  try  out  in  the  schools  something  they  had 
learned  during  the  session.  If  the  topic  had  been  music,  for  example,  they 
were  asked  to  lead  a  small  group  in  a  song  or  a  dance. 

"I  thought  we  were  just  going  to  learn  from  Mary  Lane,"  says  one 
mother,  whose  views  of  her  experiences  during  the  course  were  typical. 
"And  that  would  have  been  fine.  But  we  learned  also  from  each  other. 
One  of  us  would  have  a  problem  with  children,  and  we'd  lay  it  in  front  of 
everyone,  and  people  would  come  up  with  some  very  good  ideas.  You 
learn  ten  times  more  with  people  sitting  around  a  table  with  you.  You  let 
all  your  feelings  out,  and  everybody  gains." 

The  course  was  so  popular  that  next  year  Dr.  Lane  offered  it  again. 
And,  to  meet  the  demand  from  working  mothers,  Mrs.  Lewis  offered  a 
similar  course  in  the  evening,  which  was  attended  by  a  few  fathers  as  well 
as  by  mothers.  All  in  all,  about  35  parents  took  one  of  the  courses  and 
earned  credits  from  the  extension  division  of  San  Francisco  State  for 
doing  so.  The  credits  were  appreciated  particularly  by  half  a  dozen  of  the 
mothers  who  wanted  to  work  in  day-care  centers  and  had  to  meet  the 
requirements  for  a  license. 


Developing  Se!f-Esteem 

In  the  beginning,  NICE  made  no  effort  to  develop  a  cross-cultural 
curriculum  for  its  children.  Since  the  project  was  serving  a  cross-cultural 
population  and  had  a  cross-cultural  staff,  it  assumed  that  the  curriculum 
was  bound  to  be  cross-cultural.  The  staff  put  its  conscious  emphasis  on 
developing  skills-perceptual,  motor,  cognitive-and  trust,  autonomy, 
initiative,  and  social  competence. 

Because  many  of  the  children  had  culturally  poor  backgrounds,  the 
project  did  place  more  than  the  usual  emphasis  on  building  self- 
esteem-on  helping  each  child  see  himself  as  someone  special  and  as  a 
member  of  a  special  family.  The  staff  followed  a  number  of  fairly 
common  procedures,  such  as  hanging  a  full-length  mirror  low  enough  for 
children  to  see  themselves,  using  the  children's  names  in  talking  and 
singing,  and  identifying  and  exhibiting  work  produced  by  the  children. 
Going  farther,  the  teachers  also  gave  special  attention  to  children  with 
markedly  low  self-esteem,  sometimes  even  assigning  one  staff  member  to 
care  exclusively  for  one  child.  For  example,  a  student  teacher  was  assigned 
to  Buddy,  who  was  too  fearful  to  talk  or  even,  at  snack  time,  to  reach  out 
for  a  glass  of  fruit  juice.  The  teacher  took  him  on  walks,  read  to  him, 
played  with  him.  Half  a  year  later,  Buddy  was  talking  a  blue  streak  but 
articulating  poorly,  so  the  staff  guided  him  and  his  mother  to  the  college's 
Communication  Disorders  Clinic.  By  the  end  of  the  year,  Buddy's  self- 
concept  had  improved  dramatically. 

16 


For  deepening  and  expanding  a  child's  good  feelings  about  himself  and 
his  family,  NICE  regards  photography  as  its  most  important  activity.  Each 
school  had  a  simple  camera,  which  the  teaching  staff  was  encouiaged  to 
use  for  portraits,  candid  shots,  and  group  activity  pictures.  Since  the 
project  had  access  to  a  dark  room  at  the  College,  processing  was  in- 
expensive. The  schools  usually  had  many  pictures  on  display.  In  one 
school,  children's  photographs  were  used  to  identify  lockers;  in  another, 
each  child  had  his  own  bulletin  board,  on  which  weie  displayed  his 
photograph,  samples  of  his  work,  and  group  photographs  of  classmates. 
All  the  schools  displayed  family  pictures.  The  photographs  were  often  the 
subject  of  discussion  among  children,  teachers,  and  parents. 

Special  events  such  as  birthdays,  picnics,  and  trips  were  photographed 
and  the  prints  put  on  display  for  use  as  learning  materials.  And  occasional- 
ly pictures  taken  a  year  or  two  earlier  would  be  brought  out  for  comment. 
"When  a  child  has  a  opportunity  to  see  himself  in  this  way  through  time 
and  space,"  Dr.  Lane  comments,  "He  views  himself  as  a  changing  person. 
We  feel  he  gains  a  sense  of  openness  about  himself  and  his  potential.  These 
are  important  factors  in  the  self-concept." 

During  the  second  year,  families,  teaching  staff,  and  children 
cooperated  to  produce  a  "Self  Book"  for  each  child.  This  was  a  collection 
of  photographs  that  the  child  wanted  to  have  in  his  own  book,  together 
with  his  own  comments  about  them.  Each  book  began  with  a  photogiaph 
of  the  child  and  a  typed  transcript  of  the  story  he  dictated  about  himself. 
Then  came  other  photographs-usually  of  members  of  the  child's  family, 
his  teachers,  and  his  favorite  toys.  The  photographs  at  home  had  been 
taken  by  someone  from  the  school  in  accordance  with  the  child's  wishes. 
With  each  picture  was  a  story  the  child  had  dictated.  The  Self  Books  were 
kept  in  school.  Often  a  child  would  ask  a  teacher  or  a  parent  to  read  to 
him  from  his  book;  he  would  listen  delightedly.  From  this  project,  Dr. 
Lane  is  sure,  the  child  learned  not  only  that  his  family  was  something 
special  but  also  that  someone  cared  enough  about  him  to  record  what  he 
said. 

Later,  staff  members  and  parents  under  the  guidance  of  Mary  Lane 
produced  for  each  child  "My  NICE  ABC  Book,"  which  opened  with  his 
photograph  and  included,  under  F,  a  photograph  of  his  family.  Each  of 
the  other  pages  had  a  verse  about  one  of  the  letters  of  the  alphabet, 
together  with  photographs  of  the  children  or  teachers  whose  first  name 
began  with  that  letter.  Thus  a  child  could  learn  the  ABC's  and  at  the  same 
time  the  names  of  all  those  who  shared  with  him  three  years  in  the 
nursery  school. 


A  Cross-Cultural  Program 

It  was  apparent  from  the  start  that  some  of  the  children  noticed  dif- 
ferences in  one  another,  for  they  would  feel  another  child's  hair  as 
through  to  verify  its  differentness,  and  touch  and  comment  on  each 

17 


other's  skin.  Late  during  the  second  year,  when  the  children  weic  ap- 
proaching their  fourth  birthdays,  the  teachers  noticed  that  they  were 
becoming  more  consciously  aware  of  differences,  or  at  least  talking  moic 
often  about  them.  One  noon  as  one  of  the  black  mothers  was  leaving 
school  with  her  daughter-after  a  session  in  which  the  music  hour  had 
included  a  song  about  black  being  beautiful— the  little  girl  said :  "Mommy, 
you  and  I  are  white,  aren't  we?"  She  was  one  of  seveial  black  childicn 
who  identified  rather  strongly  with  one  or  another  of  the  white  teachers. 
On  the  othei  hand,  one  of  the  white  boys  kept  telling  his  mother  he 
wished  he  were  "dark,  like  Michael,"  because  he  liked  Michael  very  much. 

So  the  staff  began  thinking  perhaps  it  should  be  paying  more  conscious 
attention  to  the  cross-cultural  elements  of  the  ciiiriculiun.  The  feeling  was 
that  children  should  not  only  become  aware  of  differences  between  one 
ethnic  or  cultural  group  and  anothei  but  also  come  to  accept  and  ap- 
preciate these  differences.  In  addition,  a  child's  image  of  himself  and  Ins 
family  should  be  tied  to  that  of  his  group.  As  Dr.  Lane  puts  it:  "We  hoped 
each  child  would  come  to  feel  that  ll  am  Bobby  Lewis.  My  folks  are  Uic 
Lewis  family.  And  the  Lewises  are  —  black,  white,  Oriental,  mixed,  or 
whatever.1  " 

Parents  were  urged  to  attend  staff  meetings  to  talk  about  these  goals 
and  what  more  might  be  done  to  attain  them  The  parents  were  to  consti- 
tute the  Parent  Advisoiy  Council.  Only  nine  accepted,  but  these  were 
outspoken  in  their  comments.  For  example: 

"There  are  people  I  don't  want  him  to  identify  with,  no  matter  what 
color  they  are-people,  you  know,  of  lower  character." 

"How  can  you  explain  to  your  child  that  you  get  angry  with  people 
in  high  office  and  that  you  don't  like  a  ceitain  person  even  if  he  is 
President?" 

"Yeah,  it's  hard  to  build  respect,  especially  in  a  black  home.  They're 
always  talking  about  bad  white  people-they  do  this  to  us,  and  they 
did  that,  and  we're  going  to  get  'em.  Like  she  knows  somebody  white 
killed  Martin  Luther  King,  but  she's  kept  her  white  friends." 

"You  know,  it's  a  funny  thing,  I  never  even  thought  of  it  that  way 
myself-a  white  man  killed  Dr.  King,  but  I  guess  it's  a  way  of 
thinking  about  it.  I  think  I  would  say  it  was  an  individual  not  a  while 
or  black  man,  and  it's  wiong  no  matter  who  did  it." 

"I  think  the  children  need  to  know  blacks  do  bad  things  and  they  do 
good  things,  just  like  white  people  and  pink  people.  But  I  think  when 
my  little  girl  sees  a  black  do  bad  things  she  thinks  it  too  bad." 

"Well,  I  want  my  kid  to  feel  good  about  herself  and  if  she  does  that, 
maybe  she  won't  have  to  hate  other  people." 

18 


All  of  the  parents  were  kept  informed  of  the  Council's  discussions.  The 
attending  parents,  as  well  as  some  of  the  others,  contributed  ideas  for  the 
third  year's  program.  In  the  end,  the  staff  had  many  suggestions  for  ways 
of  teaching  differences,  particularly  in  matteis  of  food,  dress,  games, 
customs,  and  holidays.  Through  its  own  research  it  also  had  a  list  of  books 
and  anothei  of  recorded  music  and  songs,  from  many  cultures,  considered 
appropriate  for  prekindergartners. 

As  the  result,  the  third-year  program  included: 

•  Development  of  a  multicultural    calendar  that  marked  such  dates  as 
Martin  Luther  King's  birthday,  Chinese  New  Year,  Japanese  Children's 
Day,  and  Jewish  and  Christian  holy  days.  The  calendar  was  intended 
primarily  for  home  use,  but  some  of  the  special  days  were  observed  in 
the  schools. 

•  Visits  to  the  library  during  Negro  History  Week  to  sec  an  exhibit  of 
photographs  of  people  around  the  world. 

•  Showing  of  a  moving  picture  about  a  black  boy  and  a  white  boy 
attempting  to  span  an  ocean  inlet  by  building  a  biidge  together.  ("Bridge 
Tomorrow,"    written,    filmed,   and    produced    by  Oscar  Williams,   San 
Francisco  State  College.) 

•  Displays  of  photographs  of  prominent  people-white,  black,  Oriental. 

•  Games  and  songs  from  different  cultures. 

•  Exchange  of  recipes  among  the  mothers  and  the  sharing  of  charac- 
teristic dishes-such  as  chittlings,  teriyaki,  sweet  and  sour  pork-at  the 
weekly  school  lunches. 


Services  for  the  Family 

The  teachers  frequently  encountered  problems  that  could  not  be 
handled  with  simply  a  few  minutes  of  advice.  For  instance,  a  mother  was 
sick,  and  her  family  needed  homemaker  assistance;  a  marriage  was  on  the 
rocks;  a  family  had  to  move  but  could  find  no  place  to  move  to;  a  credit 
agency  was  threatening  suit;  an  older  child  was  in  trouble  with  the  police. 
Particularly  during  the  first  half  year  teachers  often  spent  part  of  their 
afternoons,  which  had  been  set  aside  for  planning  and  report-writing,  and 
even  some  of  their  free  time,  consulting  with  such  families  and  bringing 
them  together  with  the  appropriate  community  services. 

The  addition  to  the  staff  of  a  psychiatric  social  worker  eased  the  sit- 
uation. He  acted  as  a  consultant  to  the  teachers  and  a  counselor  and 
advocate  of  the  families  requiring  help.  With  the  aid  of  two  graduate 
students  in  social  work,  he  studied  the  public  resources  of  the  area- 
health,  employment,  legal  aid,  and  the  like— and  told  people  about  them 
in  a  meeting  to  which  the  parents  in  particular  were  invited  but  which  was 
open  to  all,  A  number  of  the  poorer  families  hadn't  known  what  was 
available  to  them  almost  next  door,  free. 

During  the  three  years  of  the  NICE  schools,  the  project  helped  3  5  families 
to  deal  with  specific  mental  health  problems  through  counseling  by  the 
social  work  staff  or  action  by  other  community  agencies,  or  both.  About 

19 


two-thirds  of  the  cases  were  child  behavior  problems,  half  of  them 
involving  nursery  school  children  and  the  other  half,  oldei  brotheis  and 
sisters. 

In  other  ways,  too,  the  pioject  became  involved  with  a  family's  older 
children.  For  boys  whose  parents  said  they  needed  something  to  do,  it 
organized  a  club  led  by  students  in  educational  sociology  at  San  Francisco 
State.  One  activity  was  going  to  baseball  games  with  their  fatheis.  If  a 
boy's  father  couldn't  attend,  the  boy  went  with  another  father  in  the 
project.  A  black  boy  might  be  picked  up  by  a  white  father,  a  white  boy  by 
a  black  one.  Other  activities  included  trips  on  foot  or  by  bus  to  interesting 
places  in  the  city.  For  older  girls,  the  project  formed  the  Girls'  Friendship 
Group,  which  met  weekly  with  a  student  leader.  For  a  dozen  older 
children  who  were  having  trouble  in  school,  it  procured  tutoring  by 
student  teachers.  During  the  project's  last  summer,  the  social  work  staff 
arranged  for  19  youngsters  to  attend  good,  small  camps. 

In  addition,  the  schools  provided  intervention  almost  continuously  and 
as  a  matter  of  course  as  the  teachers  identified  special  needs.  If  a  child 
always  hung  back  from  group  activities,  he  was  encouraged  in  a  number  of 
ways  to  join  in.  If  a  child  lacked  a  male  model  at  home,  he  was  chosen 
more  often  than  others  to  go  with  the  man  teacher  to  buy  fish  for  the 
acquarium  or  food  for  the  guinea  pigs.  If  a  child  just  sat  and  looked  blank, 
though  his  hearing  was  normal,  teachers  made  sure  that  some  smiling, 
interested  person  was  near  him,  often  talking  to  him.  Because  staff 
members  knew  children  and  families  so  well,  the  project  estimates  that  all 
but  half  a  dozen  families  received  special  help  directly  related  to  menial 
health. 

The  project  was  concerned  with  physical  health  as  well,  the  purpose 
being  to  see  that  each  family  knew  how  to  get  the  medical  services  it 
needed.  To  this  end,  the  project  required  annual  physical  examinations  Tor 
both  children  and  mothers,  and  when  necessary  it  guided  the  families  to 
facilities  where  the  examinations-and  any  treatment  required-could  be 
obtained  at  little  cost.  It  won  the  cooperation  of  the  Health  Dcparlmcnl 
in  providing  vision,  hearing,  and  dental  screening  services.  It  arranged  for  a 
comprehensive  serology  test  of  all  the  children,  primarily  to  screen  Cor 
sickle  cell  anemia,  which  has  a  relatively  high  incidence  among  black 
people.  Several  children  were  found  to  have  the  condition  and  (heir 
families  were  helped  to  get  the  necessary  medical  care.  The  projccl 
arranged  also  for  parents  to  participate  in  several  discussions  of  cancer. 

Other  activities  included: 

Swimming  classes  for  nursery  school  children  and  their  older  brothers 
and  sisters. 

A  dancing  class  for  mothers. 

Swimming  parties  for  mothers. 

A  sewing  class  for  mothers,  with  aid  and  instruction  from  home 
economic  majors  at  San  Francisco  State. 

An  art  class  for  mothers  and  fathers. 

Moving  pictures  on  family  life  and  education. 

20 


A  monthly  meeting  for  mothers,  where  the  topics  discussed  included 
"Piejudices  and  How  to  Discourage  Them"  and  "What  Advice  Would  You 
Give  to  an  18-Year-Old  Mother-To-Be?" 

School  and  tri-school  suppeis  and  picnics. 


Involvement  in  the  Community 

The  project  was  alert  for  ways  to  facilitate  the  involvement  of  families 
in  community  affairs.  One  opportunity  occurred  early  when  the  play  yard 
of  the  West  Side  Courts  Nursery  School  was  subjected  to  vandalism, 
including  the  partial  burning  of  a  playhouse.  At  a  meeting  of  the  residents 
of  this  public  housing  unit,  quickly  ananged  by  the  staff,  people  who 
were  not  members  of  the  project  had  an  opportunity  to  voice  hostility 
toward  the  school  for  serving  "white  kids"  as  well  as  black.  And  parents 
with  children  in  the  school  had  an  opportunity  to  explain  the  goals  of  the 
project  and  thus  win  for  it  greater  acceptance  by  their  neighbors. 

Beyond  that,  the  discussion  brought  out  a  legitimate  grievance:  the  lack 
of  recreational  facilities,  particularly  for  children  from  9  through  13,  who 
seemed  to  have  done  most  of  the  damage.  Through  the  efforts  of  the  head 
teacher,  a  second  meeting  was  attended  by  the  staff  of  a  nearby  neighbor- 
hood center,  and  arrangements  were  made  to  bring  Westside  Courts 
youngsteis  into  that  center's  recreational  program.  As  an  outgrowth  of 
this  experience,  staff  and  parents  worked  with  community  leaders  to 
obtain  better  planning  for  the  use  of  a  small  park  for  the  Westside  Courts 
area  and  became  more  involved  in  the  Police  Community  Relations  Pro- 
gram. Also,  one  of  the  teachers  at  the  nursery  school  started  a  wood- 
working class  for  older  boys  and  this  developed  into  a  club  for  young 
teenagers,  girls  as  well  as  boys.  The  burned  play  house,  incidentally,  was 
rebuilt  and  the  other  damage  repaired  by  a  group  of  the  fathers-black, 
white,  and  yellow-in  the  course  of  a  "work  Saturday." 

During  interviews  with  staff  members  before  the  nursery  schools 
opened,  many  of  the  parents-of  all  the  ethnic  and  socioeconomic  groups 
represented— expressed  concern  about  the  area's  public  school  facilities 
and  programs.  So  a  Public  Education  Committee  of  parents  and  staff 
members  was  set  up.  It  arranged  for  meetings  at  which  school  issues  were 
explained  and  discussed;  organized  a  program  of  parent-school  visits, 
under  which  parents  visited  the  kindergartens  their  children  would  attend; 
and  sent  representatives  to  meetings  of  the  city's  School  Board.  The 
Committee  became  an  area-wide  group. 

One  mother-black,  six  children,  on  welfare-represented  NICE  as  a 
board  member  of  the  Mental  Health  Consortium,  formed  to  work  for 
better  use  of  the  area's  mental  health  facilities.  Another  participated  in  a 
charrette,  a  community-based  meeting  open  to  the  expression  of  the 
residents'  wishes  and  complaints,  organized  by  the  Far  West  Educational 
Laboratory,  The  social  worker  represented  NICE  on  a  community  plan- 
ning group,  where  he  helped  develop  a  program  for  the  sick  children  of 
working  parents. 

21 


Breaking  Down  Boundaries 

Several  months  after  the  schools  opened,  a  white  couple  invited  all  the 
other  parents  from  one  of  the  schools  to  a  party  in  their  home  in  St. 
Francis  Square.  It  was  the  first  parents'  party.  One  black  couple  from 
public  housing  decided  not  to  go  because  they  could  not  believe  they 
would  be  welcome.  Another  got  as  far  as  the  host's  door.  "If  anybody 
looks  funny  at  me  when  they  open  the  door  and  see  who's  here,"  said 
the  man  to  his  wife,  "I'm  turning  around."  Today,  according  to  staff 
members,  the  idea  of  rejection  would  never  occur  to  any  of  the  parents, 
because  they  have  come  to  trust  one  another. 

The  development  of  trust  was  a  rather  slow  process.  Until  the  schools 
had  been  in  operation  a  year,  there  was  little  mingling  by  the  parents 
across  either  racial  01  income  lines.  The  mixing  process  speeded  up  during 
the  first  summer,  when  more  of  the  mothers  were  helping  at  the  schools 
and  getting  to  know  one  another  better,  and  whole  families  were  being 
thrown  together  at  school  picnics.  During  the  final  two  years,  middle- 
income  parents,  who  were  predominantly  white,  habitually  picked  up 
low-income  parents,  who  were  predominately  black,  and  took  them  to 
parents*  meetings  and  other  activities.  And  some  of  the  women  with  a  car 
in  the  family  organized  car  pools  so  that  working  mothers,  generally  from 
low-income  families,  would  be  saved  the  trouble  of  getting  their  children 
to  and  from  school. 

Comments  from  parents  tell  this  part  of  the  story  best. 
One  mother  who  had  never  associated  with  white  people  ("oh,  we  had 
little  friendships  in  school  but  it  was  always  kept  in  school,  you  know-- 
they  didn't  come  to  visit  me  at  home  and  I  didn't  go  to  visit  them  ut 
home")  found  herself  liking  almost  eveiybody  she  met  in  the  project. 
"And,  someway,  it's  not  a  forced  like:  I've  gotten  to  know  them  and  it's  a 
real  friend  ship- not  just  having  something  to  talk  about,  mostly  school. 
They  like  me,  and  I  like  them."  She  and  her  children  go  to  dinner  at  white 
families'  homes  and  the  white  families  and  their  children  go  to  dinner  at 
her  home. 

"We  are  meeting  people  on  a  social  level  that  we  wouldn't  have  met 
before,  except  possibly  on  a  business  level,"  said  a  professional  man  of 
Chinese  ancestry.  "This  is  the  most  magnificent  part  of  it."  He  recalled  the 
first  time  he  and  his  wife  had  been  invited  to  a  social  occasion-tin  an- 
niversary-at  the  home  of  one  of  the  black  families.  The  father  managed  n 
clothing  store.  "The  thing  that  struck  us  very  profoundly,"  said  the 
professional  man,  "was  that  these  people  have  as  much  capability  as  I 
have,  yet  they  are  not  able  to  live  nearly  so  well  for  their  efforts  as  I  am. 
And  there  are  many  black  families  who  live  even  less  well-in  one  of  those 
very  inhuman  monoliths  that  architects  have  created  to  house  those 
families.  When  you  get  them  that  kind  of  housing,  how  can  you  expect 
high-class  citizenship  performance?" 

Said  the  wife  of  a  lawyer:  "We  didn't  go  out  of  our  way  to  solicit 
friends  among  the  parents,  just  as  we  don't  go  out  of  our  way  to  solicil 
friends  among  our  neighbors.  It  was  just  a  natural  happening,  and  it  took 

22 


time.  It's  genuine."  Her  husband  noticed  a  couple  of  plainclothesmen 
looking  on  at  a  fund-raising  dance  given  by  the  new  Cross-Cultural  Family 
Center.  "It  must  seern  odd  to  them,"  he  remarked,  "this  peculiai  mix  of 
people,  having  a  good  time,  drinking  and  dancing  and  talking  and 
laughing."  His  wife  reported:  "It  was  really  beautiful." 

Another  mother,  asked  what  she  thought  of  cross-cultuial  education, 
said:  "I  think  it's  wonderful,  because  I  think  that's  the  answer  to  every- 
thing— all  the  problems  that  we're  having  now.  The  more  you  are  together 
the  more  you  really  know  it's  no  mystery....  You  find  out  that  people  are 
actually  doing  the  same  things  and  having  the  same  type  problems, 
regardless.  You  really  find  that  out." 

One  woman  said  that  even  before  NICE  she  had  been  acquainted  with  a 
number  of  people  different  from  her.  "But  I  just  didn't  have— I'd  say 
'Yeah,  it's  a  nice  day,'  and  I'd  be  finished."  She  mentioned  a  nursery 
school  mother  who  had  been  born  in  Japan.  "One  day  I  met  her  in  the 
wash  house,  and  we  started  talking.  She  was  the  sort  of  person  I  could  say, 
'It's  a  nice  day,'  and  we  were  finished— after  that— I  don't  know  what  it 
was.  I  think  it  was  a  change  in  both  of  us.  The  races  don't  make  too  much 
difference— it  depends  on  how  they  act.  That  should  be  a  thing  we  should 
remember.  She  talks  about  the  classes  where  people  were  talking  about 
how  they  grew  up  and  what  happened  to  them— their  way.  And  I  sort  of 
look  at  a  lot  of  people  and  say,  'Yes,  that's  how  you  become  you  are  what 
you  are,'  And  that  helps.  It  just  helps  in  knowing  a  lot  of  people  in 
general."  This  mother  said  she  was  joining  the  Family  Center  because  "I 
might  learn  to  know  somebody  else  that  I  didn't  know  this  year  at  all." 


The  New  Family  Center 

The  nursery  schools  closed  in  the  summer  of  1969,  as  scheduled,  and 
that  fall  the  Cross-Cultural  Family  Center  opened  as  the  successor  to 
NICE.  The  Parent  Advisory  Council  and  the  project  staff  had  spent  half  a 
year  planning  for  it. 

The  two  biggest  problems  had  been  where  to  locate  the  Center,  since 
none  of  the  school  sites  was  adequate,  and  how  to  finance  it,  since  NIMH 
was  unable  to  continue  support.  The  site  problem  was  solved 
providentially.  Some  of  the  nursery  school  parents  appeared  on  a 
television  program,  dealing  with  battered  children,  and  told  what  they  had 
learned  from  NICE  about  child-rearing  and  children's  education. 
Impressed,  members  of  the  Unitarian  Church,  which  had  recently  finished 
building  a  new  education  center,  asked  what  the  project  was  going  to  do 
next,  and  offered  the  use  of  classrooms,  play  space,  and  an  auditorium. 
The  church  representatives  explained  that  NICE,  more  than  some  other 
worthy  organizations  which  had  been  considered,  seemed  particularly  able 
to  carry  out  the  church's  commitment  -  to  use  its  new  plant  for  the  good 
of  the  community. 

Some  members  of  the  project  staff  were  not  eager  to  accept.  Dr.  Lane, 
notably,  felt  that  being  in  a  church  might  put  a  damper  on  activities.  She 

23 


also  felt  that  the  superioi,  almost  luxurious,  facilities  being  offered  might 
intimidate  some  of  the  persons  who  would  be  using  them.  She  expressed 
herself  at  a  meeting  of  the  Parent  Advisory  Council  and  suggested  that  the 
search  for  a  location  be  continued.  The  Council  voted  her  down.  One 
member  remarked:  "It  won't  take  me  long  to  get  used  to  a  little  luxury.11 
Dr.  Lane  soon  felt  altogether  happy  with  the  decision. 

The  financial  problem  was  tougher.  For  the  first  year  it  was  met  largely 
through  contributions  of  time  and  talent  by  parents  and  by  members  of 
the  original  project  staff  from  San  Francisco  State,  and  through  money 
raised  by  cake  and  rummage  sales  and  by  benefits  -  a  dance,  a  concert,,! 
fashion  show.  The  fund-raising  effoits  were  so  extensive  and  exhausting 
that  for  1970-71  the  Center  decided  to  operate  the  school  in  two  sessions, 
morning  and  afternoon,  and  to  admit  children  of  non-members  and  charge 
tuition  for  them.  Fund-raising  efforts  are  still  necessary  but  should  be  less 
arduous.  Some  members  of  the  Center  are  opposed  to  this  direction  but 
have  been  unable  to  suggest  a  viable  alternative. 

The  head  teacher  of  the  Center's  nursery  school  was  an  assistant  teacher 
in  the  original  project,  and  her  assistants  are  mothers  who  were  trained  as 
part  of  that  project.  The  Center's  activities  include  a  kindergnrten- 
supplement  program  for  the  oiiginal  NICE  children  three  times  a  week; 
individual  tutoring  in  the  afternoon  for  elementary  school  children  who 
need  help  in  order  to  keep  up  with  their  classmates;  afternoon  classes  in 
sculpting,  dancing,  and  science  for  children  of  6  and  older;  and  evening 
classes  in  cooking  and  sewing  for  children  of  10  and  older.  For  boys  and 
girls  from  7  to  10,  there  are  clubs  that  meet  weekly.  For  adults,  there  arc 
discussion  groups,  pot-luck  suppers,  occasional  socials. 

The  Parent  Advisory  Council  for  the  NICE  schools  became  the  Board  of 
Trustees  of  the  Family  Center,  which  is  a  non-profit  corporation.  The 
chairman  of  the  board,  a  man  of  Chinese  ancestry,  says:  "The  three  years 
of  the  nursery  schools  simply  started  something  that  we  expect  to  grow 
to  become  increasingly  more  beautiful."  Another  member,  a  black 
woman,  adds:  "The  Family  Center  speaks  stronger  and  louder  about  the 
value  of  NICE  than  anything  we  can  say." 

Research  Grant.  MH  14782 

Dates  of  Interviews-  November  1 969  and  September  1 970 


24 


Investigators. 1 

Msgr.  Paul  H.  Furfey,  Ph.D. 

Thomas  J.  Harte,  Ph.D. 

The  Bureau  of  Social  Research 

Catholic  University  of  America 

Washington,  D.C. 

Prepared  by. 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Introduction  and  Summary 

During  the  first  1 5  or  1 8  months  of  life,  a  number  of  investigators  have 
reported,  children  from  one  racial  or  socioeconomic  group  score  about  the 
same  in  tests  of  intellectual  functioning  as  those  from  another.  By  school 
time,  though,  different  groups  have  reached  different  levels;  in  particular, 
children  from  families  of  low  socioeconomic  status  have  a  lowei  average 
IQ  than  children  from  families  of  higher  status.  The  schools  do  not  change 
this  difference. 

Other  studies  have  shown  that  intellectual  level,  as  measured  by  mental 
tests,  is  closely  related  to  verbal  ability,  as  measuied  by  tests  of  vocabu- 
lary and  information,  and  that  disadvantaged  groups  score  lower  on  these 
verbal  tests  than  on  others.  It  has  also  been  shown  that  the  children  in 
these  groups  receive  less  verbal  stimulation  from  parents-through  being 
talked  to,  read  to,  taken  on  trips,  etc. -than  children  in  middle-class 
groups,  and  that  the  parents  are  usually  not  very  good  examples  for 
children  to  follow  in  learning  language. 

These  findings  suggest  that: 

•  Children  from    low  socioeconomic  groups  develop  deficits  in  intel- 
lectual functioning  because  they  lack  adequate  intellectual,  particularly 
verbal,  stimulation. 

•  One    reason    many    disadvantaged    families    remain    disadvantaged, 
generation  after  generation,  is  the  lower  ability  of  their  children  to  profit 
from  our  educational  system  and,  therefore,  a  lower  ability  as  adults  to 
compete  in  our  economic  system. 

•  If  adequate  stimulation  can  be  provided   early  enough,  it  should 
prevent  deficits  in  intellectual  functioning  and  thereby  help  break  the 
cycle  of  poverty  and  cultural  deprivation. 


1  This  research  was  planned  by  Earl  S.  Schaefer,  Ph.  D.  of  NIMH  and  carried  out  in 
consultation  with  him.  Doctor  Furfey  served  as  project  director  as  well  as  Co-Principal 
Investigator. 

25 


To  test  one  means  of  providing  such  stimulation,  Earl  Schaefer,  Ph.D. 
of  NIMH's  Center  for  Studies  of  Children  and  Family  Mental  Health, 
initiated  the  Infant  Education  Research  project  in  1965.  This  is  a  pioject 
guided  and  financed  by  the  Institute  but  carried  out  by  a  staff  directed  by 
Msgi.  Paul  H.  Furfey,Ph.D.  icsearch  associate,  Bureau  of  Social  Rcscaich, 
Catholic  University.  Under  it  individual  tutoring-an  hour  a  day,  5  days  a 
week-was  provided  to  a  group  of  28  Negro  boys  from  slum  aicas  of 
Washington,  D.C. 

The  tutoring  began  when  the  youngsteis  were  15  months  old  and 
continued  for  21  months-the  period  in  which  differences  between  groups 
in  their  average  level  of  intellectual  functioning  first  appear,  and  also  the 
period  of  early  verbal  development.  The  tutors  were  young  women,  who 
talked  to  the  childien,  read  to  them,  played  with  them,  brought  them  toys 
and  puzzles  and  picture  books,  took  them  on  walks  and  trips-in  short, 
tried  to  provide  the  kinds  of  mental  stimulation  necessary  to  a  child's 
development  but  not  commonly  found  among  the  most  disadvanlaged 
families.  The  tutors  dealt  with  emotional  and  behavioral  problems  as  well. 
When  the  children  were  3,  most  of  them  were  enrolled  in  nursery  schools, 
and  monthly  discussion  groups  were  set  up  for  the  mothers.  Serving  as 
contiols  were  30  Negro  boys  from  similar  neighborhoods,  who  received 
the  same  tests  as  the  others  but  were  not  tutored. 

The  investigators  hoped  to  learn  whether  or  not  this  kind  of  experience 
between  the  ages  of  15  months  and  3  years  makes  a  difference  in  intel- 
lectual functioning  and,  if  so,  whether  or  not  the  difference  persists. 

The  results  to  date  show  that  the  project  has  indeed  favorably  affected 
the  mental  development  of  the  tutored  youngsters  for  at  least  a  time. 
When  the  infants  were  tested  at  the  age  of  14  months,  a  month  before 
tutoring  began,  both  groups  scored  above  normal  on  the  Bay  ley  Infant 
Scales,  which  are  concerned  mainly  with  sensory  and  motor  development 
and  with  alertness  and  interest.  The  children  in  the  experimental  group 
were  found  to  have  an  average  IQ  of  105;  those  in  the  control  group,  of 
108. 

At  21  months,  after  half  a  year  of  tutoring,  the  average  IQ  of  the 
experimental  group  was  97;  of  the  control  group,  90,  a  difference  sig- 
nificant at  the  .05  level  (it  would  occur  by  chance  only  5  times  in  1 00). 
The  test  this  time  had  a  number  of  verbal  and  cognitive  items,  which  are 
included  in  the  Bayley  scales  for  this  age  level.  Scores  on  such  ilcms, 
which  are  closely  linked  to  success  in  school,  are  strongly  affected  by 
environment.  Presumably  the  environment  had  affected  both  groups 
adversely,  but  6  months  of  tutoring  had  brought  the  experimental  group 
close  to  normal. 

At  27  months,  the  average  IQ  of  the  experimental  group,  as  mcnsured 
by  the  Stanford-Binet  test,  had  advanced  to  101,  and  at  36  months,  to 
106.  For  the  control  group,  the  scores  were  90  and  89.  The  difference 
between  the  groups  was  significant  each  time  at  the  .01  level.  At  4  years, 
on  the  same  test,  the  children  who  had  been  tutored  rated  exactly  normal, 
100,  as  a  group;  the  other  group  rated  90. 

26 


In  addition  to  the  IQ  test  at  36  months,  the  investigators  used: 

The  Johns  Hopkins  Perceptual  Test,  in  which  the  child  is  given  a 
form— a  triangle,  say-and  asked  to  match  it  with  one  of  several  other 
forms. 

The  Peabody  Picture  Vocabulary  Test,  in  which  the  child  is  shown 
several  pictures  at  a  time  and  asked  to  point  to  the  one  illustrating  a  given 
activity  or  object  ("show  me  running"  ***  "show  me  hoise" 
11  *  *  "show  me  vegetable"). 

A  preposition  test,  developed  by  Schaefer,  which  asks  the  child  to 
place  something  over  something  else,  behind  it,  by  it,  and  so  on. 

The  tutored  children  scored  significantly  higher  on  the  perceptual  and 
vocabulary  tests  than  the  other  children,  but  not  on  the  preposition  test. 

After  each  test,  the  children  were  rated  for  task  orientedness,  meaning 
their  attention  to  the  job  at  hand  and  their  cooperation  with  the  tester.  At 
three,  this  factor  was  found  to  correlate  with  the  test  results,  as  expected. 
More  important,  both  this  factor  and  the  test  results  were  found  to  cor- 
lelate  highly  with  the  mothers'  interest  in  the  children,  as  rated  by  the 
tutors.  Where  maternal  interest  was  rated  high— as  judged,  for  example,  by 
the  mother's  efforts  to  adequately  feed  and  clothe  the  child  and  to 
express  herself  verbally  with  him— the  child's  test  scores  were  also  likely  to 
be  high.  Mothers  rated  low  in  this  respect  were  likely  to  rate  high  on  a 
child  neglect  factor,  whose  elements  included  inadequate  care,  irregular 
meals,  inadequate  clothing,  sickness,  accidents,  and  beatings.  The  children 
of  such  mothers  tended  not  only  to  do  poorly  on  mental  tests  but  also  to 
display  behavior  marked  by  belligerence,  irritability,  and  negativism. 

Nothing  in  the  family—income,  parents'  education,  presence  or  absence 
of  the  father-correlated  with  the  intellectual  functioning  of  the  child 
except  the  mother's  interest.  At  four,  maternal  interest  no  longer  cor- 
related with  task  orientedness,  but  it  did  still  correlate  with  the  children's 
IQ. 

Just  why  the  experimental  group  dropped  from  above  normal  in  intel- 
lectual functioning  at  the  age  of  3  to  normal  (although  still  10  points 
above  the  control  group)  at  age  4  is  a  matter  of  conjecture.  Doctor 
Schaefer  thinks  it  is  because  the  tutoring  stopped,  except  for  sessions  twice 
a  month.  "It  is  quite  clear,"  he  observes,  "that  if  we  tutor  a  culturally 
deprived  child  for  21  months  we  increase  the  rate  of  his  intellectual 
growth  during  that  period.  But  it  is  not  clear  that  he  will  stay  at  the  level 
he  reaches  then  unless  the  extra  stimulation  is  continued;  the  evidence  at 
this  point  is  that  he  will  not. 

"The  question  is  not  how  to  offset  a  poor  genetic  potential,  The  project 
demonstrated  that  these  lower-class  black  children  had  the  genetic 
potential  for  an  adequate  level  of  intellectual  functioning:  with  only  5 
hours  a  week  of  stimulation,  they  scored  above  the  norms  during  the 
period  of  stimulation.  The  question  is,  how  can  we  foster  that  genetic 
potential  over  the  long  run?"  Schaefer 's  answer,  set  forth  more  fully  in  a 
later  section,  is:  by  early  and  continued  stimulation-to  be  achieved  by 
educating  future  parents  to  be  good  parents. 

27 


The  Infant  Education  Research  Project  is  continuing.  When  the  children 
are  in  kindeigarten,  between  the  ages  of  5  and  6,  tutors  will  visit  the 
homes  of  those  in  the  experimental  group  twice  a  week,  primarily  to  work 
with  the  mothers  in  the  use  of  educational  materials.  The  mothers  will 
also  be  taken  on  visits  to  the  school,  in  the  hope  of  developing  better 
relations  between  home  and  school,  and  they  will  continue  to  meet 
monthly.  Both  the  experimental  and  the  controls  will  be  tested  during 
the  first  year  of  elementary  school  to  learn  if  the  benefits  of  the  inter- 
vention program  continue.  Also,  younger  children  in  the  families  are  being 
tested  to  learn  if  there  has  been  a  carryover  from  the  tutoring.  (At  prcscril 
there  is  only  anecdotal  material.  For  example,  one  mother  spoke  of  the 
child  being  tutored  as  "your"  child  and  of  her  new  baby  as  "my"  child. 
When  the  tutor  came  to  teach  the  older  baby,  the  mother  would  sit  in  the 
next  room  and  play  with  and  talk  to  the  new  baby.  This  one  became  quite 
precocious  in  language,  speaking  in  sentences  at  the  age  of  13  months,) 

Staff  members  believe  that  a  program  based  on  the  one  reported  here 
but  not  intended  as  a  research  project  could  be  mounted  in  any  com- 
munity at  no  great  cost.  The  tutors  could  be  either  mature  women  or 
high  school  girls,  chosen  for  their  ability  to  relate  to  children  and  properly 
trained  and  supervised.  Most  of  the  recordkeeping  and  psychological 
testing  couid  be  eliminated. 

Howevei,  staff  members  also  believe,  on  the  basis  of  the  work  so  far, 
that  ways  should  be  found  of  working  with  eveiybody  in  the  family— not 
just  with  the  youngest  child  and  to  some  extent  his  mother.  If  the  whole 
family  were  brought  into  an  educational  project,  they  think,  the  older 
children  and  the  parents  would  become  more  interested  in  and  capable  of 
carrying  on  measures  that  stimulate  intellectual  growth.  So  the  project 
should  have  a  broader  and  longer  lasting  effect. 

Two  subsidiary  studies  likely  to  be  of  help  in  future  work  with  dis- 
advantaged  groups  are  under  way.  One,  by  Doctor  Furfey  and  Doctor 
Mary  Elizabeth  Walsh,  of  the  Bureau  of  Social  Research,  is  an  analysis  of 
the  children's  cultural  background,  based  on  the  tutors'  observations  of 
daily  life  in  the  homes  of  their  charges.  The  other,  by  Doctor  lidna 
O'Kern,  of  the  Department  of  Sociology,  St.  Francis  College,  New  York, 
is  an  analysis  of  the  children's  language,  based  on  their  taped  responses  to 
pictures. 


Findings  of  Earlier  Studies 

When  Schaefer,  working  with  Catholic  University's  Bureau  of  Social 
Research,  proposed  the  Infant  Education  Research  Project  in  1965,  con- 
siderable evidence  had  accumulated  that  the  intelligence  level  of  children 
in  poor  environments  could  be  raised.  The  most  dramatic  example  had 
been  reported  by  Harold  M.  Skeels  and  Harold  B.  Dye  in  1939. 

Skeels  and  Dye  studied  25  children  who  as  babies  had  been  committed 
to  an  orphanage  which  had  few  attendants  and  provided  little  stimulation. 
At  ages  running  from  7  months  to  3  years,  1 3  of  the  children-all  of  them 

28 


showing  evidence  of  marked  retardation— were  transferred  to  an  insti- 
tution for  the  mentally  handicapped  and  placed  in  wards  of  the  older  and 
brighter  women,  one  or  two  babies  to  a  ward  These  women,  with  mental 
ages  of  9  to  12,  gave  the  babies  adoring  attention.  Over  the  next  yeai  or 
two,  the  children  made  extraordinary  IQ  gains,  ranging  from  7  to  58 
points.  Most  of  the  children  were  later  placed  in  adoptive  homes.  Of  the 
12  children  in  a  contrast  group,  left  in  the  orphanage,  only  one  showed  an 
IQ  gain,  of  2  points;  the  others  had  losses  langing  from  8  to  45  points. 

In  1 966  Skeels  published  the  results  of  a  followup  study  made  while  he 
was  at  NIMH.  As  adults  all  13  persons  in  the  experimental  gioup  showed 
average  or  better  than  average  achievement  "as  indicated  by  education, 
occupation,  income,  family  adjustment,  intelligence  of  the  childien,  and 
contribution  to  the  community."  Of  the  12  persons  in  the  contrast  group, 
one  had  died  in  a  home  for  the  mentally  retarded,  four  were  in  insti- 
tutions, one  was  self-sufficient  at  a  middle-class  level,  and  the  other  six 
had  such  jobs  as  dishwasher,  part-time  cafeteria  workei,  and  assistant  to 
the  gardener  in  an  institution.  Most  or  all  of  the  12,  Skeels  believed, 
would  have  achieved  within  the  normal  range  had  they  been  placed  early 
in  infancy  in  suitable  adoptive  homes  piovided  with  equivalent  stimula- 
tion. 

Other  investigatois,  among  the  many  who  influenced  the  planning  of 
the  Infant  Education  Research  project,  had  reported. 

•  No   differences  between  Negroes  and  whites  in  mental  tests  given 
during  the  first  15  months.-Nancy  Bayley. 

•  Negro  elementary  children  in  five  southeastern  states  have  a  stable 
average  IQ  of  81  for  the  years  8  through  1  l.-W.  A.  Kennedy,  V.  Van  de 
Riet,  J.  C.  White,  Jr. 

•  Infants  whose  mothers  had  been  instructed  to  lead  and  talk  to  them 
began  at   17  months  to  produce  more  speech  sounds  than  babies  in  a 
control  group.— Orvis  C.  Irwin. 

•  The  intelligence  of  children  reared  by  foster  parents  was  more  like 
that  of  the  foster  parents  than  of  the  less  educated,  lower  socioeconomic 
true  parents.-Marie  Skodak. 

•  At    18    months,    the   IQs  of   170   London   children  were  virtually 
identical,  regardless  of  social  class.  At  3  years,  childien  in  the  upper  class 
showed  a  gain  of  about  22  points;  those  in  the  middle  class,  of  about  9 
points.  Lower-class  children  dropped  about  four  points.-C.  B.  Hindley. 

•  Groups  of  Negro  children  of  different  socioeconomic  classes  showed 
consistent  class  differences  in  language  skills. — Vera  P.  John.  Later  came 
the  finding  that  the  teaching  skills  of  160  Negro  mothers  and  the  per- 
formance of  their  4-year-old  children  differed  greatly  according  to  the 
families'  socioeconomic  level. -Robert  D.  Hess  and  Virginia  Shipman. 

•  Among  292  children,  both  lower  class  and  minority-group  status  were 
associated  with  poorer  language  functioning.— Martin  Deutsch, 

•  Fifteen  children  committed  to  a  State  institution  as  feebleminded  and 
given  a  special  2-year  training  program  showed  an  average  gain  2  years 
later  of  10,2  points  on  the  Stanford-Binet  IQ  test  and  10.5  points  on  the 
Vineland  Social  Maturity  Scale.  Children  in  a  control  group  dropped  6.5 

29 


points  on  the  IQ  test  and  12  points  on  the  social  maturity  scale;  in  other 
words  they  were  more  retarded  at  eight  than  they  had  been  at  four. 
Follow-ups  showed  that  six  of  the  first  group  but  none  of  the  second  had 
been  permanently  paroled  from  the  institution. -S.  A.  Kirk. 

•  All  gifted  children  apparently  have  received  intensive  early  stimula- 
tion. This  special  stimulation  may  be  indispensable  to  the  development  of 
high  abilities.  There  is  no  evidence  the  children  suffered  ill  effects  of  any 
kind. -William  Fowler. 

•  "*  *  *  it  appears  that  the  counsel  from  experts  on  child-rearing  dining 
the  third  and  much  of  the  fourth  decades  of  the  twentieth  century  to  let 
children  be  while  they  giow  and  to  avoid  excessive  stimulation  was  highly 
unfortunate.  *  *  *  It  is  no  longer  unreasonable  to  consider  that  it  miglil  be 
feasible  to  discover  ways  to  govern  the  encounters  that  children  have  with 
their  environments,  especially  during  the  early  years  of  their  development, 
to  achieve  a  substantially  faster  rate  of  intellectual  development  and  a 
substantially  highei  adult  level  of  intellectual  achievement.  *  *  *  The  fuel 
that  it  is  reasonable  to  hope  to  find  ways  of  raising  the  level  of  intcllcclmil 
capacity  in  a  majority  of  the  population  makes  it  a  challenge  to  tlo  ilic 
necessary  research."-!.  McV.  Hunt. 


Training  the  Tutors 

The  tutors  were  chosen  because  they  were  intelligent,  outgoing  women 
interested  in  working  with  children.  All  had  had  experience  in  jobs- 
teaching,  social  work,  nursing-that  brought  them  in  touch  with  childicu, 
A  few  were  mothers.  With  the  exception  of  a  woman  who  was  still  in 
college,  all  were  college  graduates.  Five  were  black  and  four  white.  They 
worked  together  beautifully,  a  project  official  reports,  and  those  of  one 
color  seemed  to  get  along  with  the  children  as  well  as  those  of  the  other 
(Each  child   had   two  tutors -generally   one  of  them  black,  the  oilier 
white— who  alternated  weekly  in  working  with  him.  This  arrangement  was 
intended  to  lessen  disruptive  effects  in  case  a  tutor  left  or  was  changed,  to 
reduce  any  feeling  on  the  part  of  the  mother  that  the  tutor  was  competing 
for  the  child's  affection,  and  to  provide  two  different  observers  of  the 
child  and  his  family.) 

During  the  initial  training  period,  which  ran  for  about  2  months,  half- 
time,  the  tutors  heard  and  discussed  lectures  by  Schaefer,  Furfey,  and 
others  on  child  development,  the  special  problems  of  the  disadvantage! 
child,  and  means  of  overcoming  those  problems .  The  need  to  develop  and 
maintain  a  relationship  that  would  foster  the  child's  interest,  happiness, 
and  success  in  his  new  experience  was  emphasized.  If  this  need  were  to  be 
met,  the  tutors  were  told  they  would  have  to  accept  the  child's  interests, 
praise  his  achievements,  and  enthusiastically  explore  new  learning  op- 
portunities with  him.  The  interaction  between  tutor  and  child  was  to  be 
playful,  spontaneous,  and  pleasant;  formal  instruction  would  be  avoided. 
A  tutor  must  be  aware  that  a  child's  early  learning  involves  a  great  denl  of 
looking,  listening,  tasting,  smelling,  and  feeling.  She  must  also  be  aware 

30 


that  she  is  a  model  for  the  child— and  the  mother-to  imitate.  She  will 
name  tilings  casually  but  often— toys,  household  objects,  pictures  in 
books,  events,  clothes,  the  child's  body. 

"We  aie  all  investigators,  researchers,  expeiimenteis  together,"  Schaefer 
told  the  tutors.  "We  are  searching  for  a  method  of  early  education  which 
will  raise  the  intellectual  functioning  of  childien  and  increase  their  ability 
to  do  well  in  our  American  school  system.  We  think  we  know  some  of  the 
characteristics  which  help  children  to  succeed  in  school.  We  think  it  helps 
a  child  to  be  cooperative,  outgoing,  verbal,  friendly,  helpful  to  others, 
resourceful,  curious,  attentive,  to  have  a  goal-directed  behavior  and  be 
able  to  concentrate  and  persevere.  It  helps  if  he  has  developed  a  feeling  of 
competence  and  of  human  worth,  if  he  is  able  to  asseit  himself  in  a 
positive  way  and  make  a  worthwhile  contribution  to  a  gioup.  It  is  of  gieat 
value  that  he  have  good  comprehension  and  a  good  vocabulary.  We  hope 
to  find  ways  of  developing  at  least  some  of  those  characteristics." 

Examples  of  a  number  of  specific  activities  that  might  be  used  during 
the  tutoring  sessions  were  discussed -among  them,  blowing  bubbles, 
playing  with  beads,  making  things  with  pipe  cleaneis,  playing  a  guessing 
game,  making  a  pull  toy,  having  fun  with  paper  plates,  and  playing  a 
sotting  game.  The  tutors  were  asked  to  encourage  the  child  to  participate 
in  any  activity  as  much  as  possible  and  to  talk  to  him  about  what  was 
going  on.  If  tutor  and  child  were  making  a  bean  bag,  for  example,  the  talk 
might  go  something  like  this: 

We  will  make  a  bean  bag. 

Please  hold  the  cloth  for  me. 

1  will  cut  it. 

Be  careful.  The  scissors  are  sharp. 

Move  your  hands. 

That's  it. 

Now  we  can  sew  it. 

Look  at  this  needle. 

See  the  tiny  hole. 

We  have  to  get  the  thread  in  this  tiny  hole-the  eye  of  the  needle. 

Look  at  my  thimble. 

Try  it  on  your  finger. 

See  how  it  fits  on  mine. 

Please  hold  the  cloth  while  I  thread  the  needle. 

Watch  me  thread  it. 

You  can  string  beads.  Some  day  you  will  be  able  to  thread  a  needle. 

Now,  I  will  hold  the  bag  open  while  you  put  the  pebbles  in. 

Put  in  one;  now  one  more. 

Put  a  whole  bunch  in  at  once  if  you  can. 

Now  we  will  sew  up  the  last  side. 

We  must  sew  it  so  no  pebbles  will  fall  out. 

See,  it's  all  done. 

Thanks  for  helping,  Johnny.  Johnny  helped  to  make  a  bean  bag. 

See,  isn't  it  pretty.  We're  all  finished, 

31 


Now  move  back,  so  I  can  throw  it  to  you,  etc. 

Now  let's  try  throwing  it  into  that  pail.  Let's  take  turns,  etc. 

Making  a  bean  bag  would  be  fun.  It  would  also  help  the  child  build  his 
vocabulary.  And  it  would  help  him  in  learning  to  count,  learning  goal- 
directed  activity,  learning  that  a  combination  of  materials  can  make 
something  different  from  the  original  materials,  and  learning  that  a  person 
can  create  something  and  then  have  fun  with  it. 

Emphasis  throughout  was  placed  on  language  stimulation.  Schaefcr 
urged  the  tutors  to  get  the  childien  "hooked  on  books." 

The  technique  of  transfer,  too,  was  emphasized.  "Children  are  happier 
and  more  comfortable  with  the  familiar,"  Schaefer  pointed  out.  "They 
can  even  become  frightened  by  something  entirely  novel.  Begin  work  with 
something  familiar  to  the  child.  If  it  is  something  he  likes  and  enjoys  you 
will  soon  be  able  to  transfer  to  a  slightly  different  use  or  activity  and  thus 
capitalize  on  the  child's  initial  inteiest  and  pleasure. 

"For  example,  if  he  finds  it  fun  to  shake  jingle  bells  because  he  enjoys 
the  sound,  you  may  latei  be  able  to  transfer  to  sorting  jingle  bells  ac- 
cording to  size  or  color,  counting  them,  making  things  with  them,  etc,, 
and  still  maintain  his  interest  and  joy,  while  at  the  same  time  stirnulaling 
new  learning." 

As  part  of  their  training,  the  tutors  observed  the  activities  of  children  in 
day  centers,  an  orphanage,  an  institution  for  delinquent  children,  and  the 
homes  of  middle-class  families  having  children  of  roughly  the  same  age  as 
those  in  the  project.  The  tutors  also  recruited  the  families  for  the  research 
program,  by  knocking  on  almost  200  doors  in  several  of  Washington's 
worst  slum  areas,  and  thus  became  acquainted  with  the  types  of  homes 
they  would  be  going  into.  And  each  worked  for  two  weeks  with  a  pilot 
case,  a  child  who  was  a  little  too  old  to  be  chosen  for  the  project. 

There  was  in-service  training,  too.  Each  tutor  discussed  cases  frequently 
with  the  leader  of  the  tutoring  team  (in  the  beginning  a  speech  therapist 
who  had  been  a  teacher  of  preschool  deaf  children,  and  since  mid-1967 
Mrs.  Lillie  Davidson,  formerly  a  nursery  school  teacher,  a  supervisor  of 
teachers,  and  a  supervisor  of  children's  counselors  in  a  center  for  homeless 
children).  Case  conferences  involving  Schaefer,  Furfey,  the  head  of  the 
tutoring  group,  the  tutors,  and  sometimes  a  mother  and  her  child  were 
held  frequently.  All  the  tutors  met  weekly  with  other  staff  members  to 
discuss  problems  and  to  give  and  receive  suggestions  about  educational 
materials  and  techniques. 


The  Children  and  Their  Families 

The  effect  of  certain  environmental  factors  upon  intellectual  function- 
ing has  been  found  to  differ  according  to  sex.2  Since  the  project  could  no! 


The  Hffect  of  Childhood  Influences  Upon  Intelligence,  Personality  and  Mental 
Health  in  Mental  Health  Program  Reports-3. 

32 


afford  a  large  enough  sample  to  study  both  sexes,  it  limited  itself  to  boys 
because  lower-class  black  boys  and  men  seem  to  have  a  more  difficult  time 
in  school  and  society  than  girls  and  women. 

The  subjects  were  chosen  from  census  tracts  selected  because  they  had 
high  rates  of  crime,  delinquency,  infant  mortality,  joblessness,  dilapidated 
housing,  and  families  on  welfare.  The  neighborhood  environment  as  well 
as  the  typical  home  environment  was  deprived.  Though  the  experimentals 
lived  in  a  slightly  worse  neighborhood  than  the  controls,  the  children  from 
one  area  had  been  scoring  much  the  same  as  those  from  the  other  on  the 
Metropolitan  Readiness  Test,  given  to  children  on  entering  school.  The 
median  scores  were  approximately  at  the  20th  percentile  on  national 
norms,  meaning  that  they  fell  in  the  lowest  fifth. 

Families  having  a  boy  of  the  right  age  were  invited  to  participate  if  the 
home  situation  was  relatively  stable-that  is,  if  it  did  not  look  too  chaotic 
for  daily  tutoring  session— and  if  the  family  met  two  of  three  other 
qualifications:  its  yearly  income  was  less  than  $5,000,  the  mother  had  not 
finished  high  school,  the  mother  had  never  been  employed  as  a  skilled 
worker. 

Among  the  98  homes  the  tutors  visited  to  select  the  experimental 
group,  12  were  rejected  by  the  staff  as  unsuitable  (because  they  were 
considered  unsafe  for  the  tutors  or  too  ciowded  to  work  in,  or  because 
the  family's  attitude  was  judged  likely  to  present  unduly  severe  problems). 
Five  other  homes  were  ruled  out  because  either  the  income  or  the 
mother's  education  was  too  high.  Most  of  the  other  rejections  were  made 
because  the  children  were  not  of  the  right  age.  Four  families  refused  to 
participate. 

The  control  group  was  selected  from  89  homes  in  a  slightly  better  area. 
Ten  were  rejected  by  the  staff  as  unsuitable,  and  12  were  ruled  out 
because  the  economic  or  educational  level  was  too  high. 

Stipends-$10  for  each  testing  session  and  $1  for  each  tutoring 
session— were  offered  primarily  in  the  hope  of  reducing  losses  from  the 
sample,  particularly  losses  of  children  with  poorly  motivated  mothers. 
Since  losses  were  expected  to  be  high  in  any  case,  and  the  project  wanted 
to  retain  at  least  20  in  each  group,  31  children  were  chosen  to  be  tutored 
and  33  to  serve  as  controls.  A  number  of  the  families  have  broken  up  and 
some  have  moved,  as  many  as  five  times.  But  only  three  in  each  group 
have  been  lost.  (Among  the  experimental  families,  one  moved  out  of 
town;  one  placed  the  baby  in  a  day  center  so  the  mother  could  take  a  job, 
and  the  center  refused  to  admit  the  tutors;  and  one  notified  the  project 
that  the  tutors  would  no  longer  be  safe:  the  wife's  mother  had  moved  in 
and  she  liked  to  throw  things.)  This  unusual  record  is  attributed  both  to 
the  interest  of  most  of  the  mothers  in  having  something  done  for  their 
children  and  to  the  tutors'  persistence  in  following  families  and  staying  on 
the  job  in  spite  of  any  unpleasant  circumstances.  The  staff  now  thinks 
that  without  the  financial  inducement,  no  more  than  three  or  four  of  the 
families  in  each  group  would  have  been  lost. 

The  homes  as  a  rule  had  neither  toys  nor  books,  and  the  mothers  at 
first  seemed  to  think  it  a  joke  to  suppose  that  anything  could  be  taught  to 

33 


a  child  as  young  as  15  months.  Some  of  the  parents,  even  some  of  those 
on  welfare  and  with  half  a  dozen  children,  were  well  organized.  The 
families  always  had  food;  the  children  were  sent  to  school  daily;  the 
mother  knew  how  to  go  to  second-hand  stores  and  buy  clothes.  Other 
parents  seemed  overwhelmed  by  their  problems. 

The  following  quick  sketches  of  representative  cases  are  drawn  from 
tutoi's  reports: 

W.  lived  with  his  parents,  a  brother,  a  sister,  an  uncle,  and  an  aunt-7 
persons  in  all-in  a  three-room  apartment  on  the  third  floor  of  a  rapidly 
deteriorating  house.  The  rats  were  so  bad,  his  mother  said,  that  she  had  to 
beat  on  a  pan  to  frighten  them  away  before  she  entered  the  kitchen.  They 
had  chewed  the  nose  off  a  doll.  The  father  had  a  steady  job  as  a  truck 
driver,  and  the  mother  woiked  part-time  as  a  counter  girl  in  a  nearby 
drugstore.  She  kept  the  apartment  tidy,  showed  her  love  for  her  children, 
and  participated  in  the  tutoring  sessions. 

At  15  months,  J.  was  a  shy,  neglected,  poorly  dressed  child  who  lived 
with  his  baby  sister  and  his  17-year-old  mother  in  a  dingy  six-room  apart- 
ment, part  of  an  old  house.  It  was  occupied  by  his  grandmother,  who 
worked  as  a  domestic,  an  unemployed  uncle,  and  the  uncle's  4-year-old 
daughter.  J.'s  father  was  in  jail.  The  apartment  was  uncrowded  but  un- 
pleasant. Fumes  from  an  oil  stove,  which  supplied  inadequate  heat,  made 
the  eyes  smart.  Chunks  of  plaster  had  fallen  from  the  walls.  The  bedroom 
floors  were  littered  with  soiled  clothing.  Cockroaches  crawled  allaiound. 
There  were  no  toys  or  books,  but  there  were  newspapers,  a  television,  a 
radio,  and  three  telephones  which  had  been  disconnected.  The  mother,  a 
high  school  dropout,  was  quiet  and  timid,  knew  little  about  raising  her 
children,  and  was  apparently  unable  to  show  affection  for  them.  She  was 
interested  mainly  in  going  to  school  and  getting  a  job.  J,  was  cared  for  by 
the  grandmother,  a  warm  and  friendly  person  and  apparently  the  only 
member  of  the  household  bringing  in  money,  and  a  babysitter, 

When  the  tutoring  began,  V.  lived  with  his  parents  in  a  spacious  one- 
bedroom  apartment  and  slept  with  them  in  a  double  bed,  His  father  had  a 
steady  job,  and  his  mother  worked  in  the  afternoon  until  her  husband 
asked  her  to  quit  so  she  could  spend  more  time  with  the  boy.  This  wns 
one  of  the  few  homes  with  books  and  toys.  V.  was  as  spoiled  as  any 
middle  class  only  child  could  be.  When  the  tutor  and  he  went  for  a  walk 
and  passed  a  store  without  going  in  and  buying  something,  he  had  a 
temper  tantrum.  One  night  some  men  came  to  the  apartment,  threatened 
V.'s  father  with  a  gun,  and  chased  him  out,  The  next  day  the  family 
moved  to  another  section  of  the  city.  This  was  near  the  end  of  the  tutor- 
ing program.  The  mother  seemed  despondent  over  the  course  of  events, 
and  the  boy  showed  hostility  toward  the  tutors. 

A.  and  his  mother,  brother,  and  sister  lived  in  a  two-bedroom  basement 
apartment  that  reeked  with  the  odor  of  urine  because  the  mother  washed 
clothes  and  diapers  together  and  did  not  properly  rinse  them.  Sometimes 
the  odor  was  so  bad  that  the  tutor  had  to  take  the  boy  outside.  Cock- 
roaches added  to  the  discomfort.  The  mother,  strict  and  brutal,  would 
punish  the  children  either  by  striking  them  or  locking  them  up  in  a  room. 

34 


E.  was  a  happy,  sociable  child  who  icadily  sought  and  gave  affection. 
He  lived  with  his  five  brothers  and  sisters  and  his  parents  in  a  two-room 
apartment,  which  needed  repaiis  but  was  kept  in  leasonable  order.  His 
mother  worked  nights  in  a  carry-out  restaurant.  She  loved  him  and  did  a 
good  job  of  caring  for  him.  E.'s  parents  separated  during  first  year  of 
tutoring,  and  the  mother  moved  with  all  the  children  to  another  two-room 
apartment.  When  E.  entered  nursery  school,  she  began  going  to  night  high 
school  so  she  could  get  a  better  job. 

R.'s  mother  showed  the  tutor  a  scar  on  the  boy's  upper  thigh  that  had 
come  from  a  rat  bite.  She  had  four  other  children  but  managed  to  sit  in  on 
many  of  the  tutoring  sessions,  often  went  along  on  trips,  and  was  proud  of 
R.'s  accomplishments.  There  were  books  in  the  house.  The  family  seemed 
to  pay  little  attention  to  R.'s  fathei,  who  did  not  work  regularly.  When 
the  mother  got  a  job  as  a  teacher's  aide  in  a  Head  Start  center,  the 
children  were  sent  to  a  baby-sitter's  home  and  the  tutoring  continued 
there. 

T.'s  mother  separated  from  her  husband  during  the  course  of  the 
project  and  moved  with  her  six  children  into  a  one-bedroom  apartment. 
Three  boys  and  their  8-year-old  sister  slept  in  a  double  bed,  another  boy  in 
a  crib,  the  newest  baby  in  a  bassinet,  and  the  mother  in  a  single  bed  in  the 
same  room.  A  bed  in  the  living  room  was  often  used  by  overnight  guests. 
The  mother,  who  seemed  disturbed  as  a  result  of  problems  first  with  hei 
husband  and  then  with  a  new  man,  constantly  screamed  at  the  chikhen 
and  threatened  them  with  belts  but  got  no  response.  The  toilet  was 
usually  broken.  Some  of  the  children  had  ringworm.  T.  kept  complaining 
of  being  hungry.  The  tutors  found  it  difficult  to  get  him  to  concentrate. 


Working  With  the  Children 

The  tutois  set  out  to  build  friendly,  easy  relationships  with  the  children 
by  playing  with  them  and  talking  to  them— activities  in  which  some  of 
the  mothers  had  engaged  only  rarely.  Some  children  made  friends  almost 
immediately.  Others  hung  back.  With  one  boy,  shy  almost  to  the  point  of 
being  withdrawn,  the  tutois  had  to  work  almost  a  year  to  establish 
rapport.  In  other  cases,  productive  relationships  developed  only  after  the 
tutors  had  worked  to  change  maternal  attitudes  and  behavior. 

At  the  start,  the  children  were  given  manipulative  toys-blocks,  pop 
beads,  nesting  sets— which1  they  learned  to  put  together  and  to  separate. 
One  toy  would  lead  to  a  number  of  activities.  Given  pop  beads,  for 
example,  a  child  would  feel  them,  swing  them  in  the  air,  and  chew  or  suck 
on  them.  Perhaps  he  would  learn  by  accident  that  they  could  be  pulled 
apart.  This  delighted  him.  Putting  them  together  was  too  difficult  for 
most  of  the  children  at  15  months,  so  the  tutor  would  show  how  to  do  it 
and  then  guide  the  child's  hands  till  he  could  do  it  himself.  Other  activities 
with  beads  included  hiding  some  of  them  around  the  room  for  the  child  to 
find  and  add  to  the  string,  using  a  long  string  of  them  as  a  pull  toy, 
counting,  naming  colors,  and  matching  according  to  color  or  shape.  Along 

35 


with  the  simple  manipulative  .toys,  the  child  was  given  opportunities  to 
play  with  balls  of  various  sizes,  a  kiddie-car,  a  pounding  bench,  and  other 
toys  that  helped  develop  the  larger  muscles. 

Always  the  tutor  talked  to  him  about  what  was  going  on.  When  ;i  new 
toy-for  instance  a  ball -was  presented,  she  would  name  it.  The  games  thai 
could  be  played  with  the  toy  gave  the  child  opportunities  to  hear,  imitate, 
and  learn  other  new  words-for  instance,  roll,  throw,  catch,  and  kick  As 
the  child  matured,  the  tutor  would  talk  about  the  different  sizes,  kinds, 
colors,  and  numbei  of  balls. 

If  a  boy  liked  a  toy  car,  the  tutor  would  talk  about  that  car  and  other 
cars.  Then  she  would  get  a  book  containing  pictures  of  cars  and  talk  with 
the  child  about  what  he  saw  on  each  page.  When  tutor  and  child  went  for 
a  walk,  they  would  talk  about  the  cars  seen.  Looking  at  picutres  of  cars  m 
a  book  helped  develop  an  inteiest  in  books,  too.  One  tutor  was  surprised 
and  pleased  to  find  the  children  associating  one  make  of  automobile  with 
her.  When  they  saw  this  auto  in  a  book  or  on  the  street,  the  response 
would  be,  "Tha1  teacher's  car!" 

Toys  were  used  not  only  to  aid  physical  development  and  to  teach 
language  but  also  to  develop  two  of  the  characteristics  essential  to  the 
mastery  of  schoolwork-attenriveness  and  perception.  Here  puzzles  were 
considered  especially  valuable.  First  came  simple,  isolated-objecl 
puzzles—for  examples  board  containing  pictures  of  three  kinds  of  fruit, 
to  be  taken  out  and  put  back  in-which  were  introduced  between  the  ages 
of  15  months  and  2  years,  More  difficult  puzzles  followed.  One  tutor, 
Betty  Pair,  describing  her  experiences  for  the  benefit  of  other  persons 
working  with  children,  wrote: 

"We  talk  about  the  puzzle  while  it  is  still  intact;  then  dump  out  the 
pieces  (this  act  I  leave  to  the  child,  because  he  seems  to  derive  great 
pleasure  from  the  dumping);  then  talk  about  the  side  with  colors  on  it  and 
the  dark,  rough  side;  then  trace  with  our  fingers  around  the  inside  of  the 
puzzle;  then  attempt  to  fit  the  pieces  in  the  puzzle. 

"It  is  important  that  the  child  complete  the  task,  but  it  is  imperative 
that  he  not  become  so  frustrated  in  his  attempt  to  do  so  that  he  sets  up  a 
negative  block  against  the  activity.  For  this  reason,  I  initially  put  the 
pieces  back  slowly  in  the  puzzle  so  that  the  child  can  observe  me.  This  is 
the  'I  Can  Do  It'  part.  We  then  see  if  he  can  do  it,  with  the  assistance  he 
may  require  to  prevent  overt  frustration.  When  there  are  signals  that 
assistance  is  required,  1  put  my  hand  over  the  child's  hand  on  the  puzzle 
piece,  and  I  explain,  'Turn  it  around,1  or  'Turn  it  over,'  or  'try  another 
space/  as  we  do  what  each  command  directs." 

Another  tutor,  Lucile  Banks,  offered  these  suggestions; 

1 .  Present  puzzles  as  enjoyable  games. 

2.  Demonstrate  how  pieces  are  placed. 

3.  Give  each  piece  a  name. 

4.  Begin  by  taking  only  one  piece  out  of  the  board  at  a  time. 

5.  Finish  one  puzzle  before  starting  another. 

36 


6.  If  frustration  persists,  direct  the  child  to  a  moie  relaxing  activity- 
such  as  painting  01  a  favorite  toy. 

7.  Praise  the  child. 

8.  Present  puzzles  already  masteied  for  relaxation  and  reinforcement. 

Through  playtime  activities,  the  tutors  also  worked  to  modify  un- 
desirable behavior.  If  a  child  were  hyperactive,  the  tutor  would  try  to  get 
him  interested  in  one  activity— like  putting  rings  on  a  stick,  arranging 
animals,  building  a  wagon-and  would  work  with  him  to  see  it  completed. 
"Well,  Bobby,"  she'd  say,  "let's  finish  this.  I  will  help  you.  *  *  *  No,  we 
aren't  going  to  play  with  that  until  we've  finished  this"  or  "until  we've 
picked  this  up  and  put  it  away." 

Toys  are  credited  with  having  aided  social  development,  because  they 
usually  had  to  be  shared  with  other  children  in  the  house,  either  during 
the  tutoring  sessions  or  between  them.  Generally  a  toy  was  left  in  the 
home  for  several  weeks.  If  it  became  a  favorite  it  might  be  left  longer,  or 
one  like  it  bought  as  a  birthday  or  Christmas  present. 

Tutors  and  mothers  worked  together  to  make  toys.  Milk  cartons-cut 
into  squares  and  the  squares  covered  with  paper-became  blocks;  oatmeal 
boxes,  drums;  pierced  bottle  caps  on  a  string,  a  tambourine.  Bleaching 
compound  bottles  were  shaped  for  use  as  dolls'  cribs.  A  roll  of  shelf  paper 
filled  with  sketches  ("This  is  Jim  *  *  *  he  lives  in  a  house  *  *  *  he  has  3 
brothers  *  *  *")  became,  as  it  was  slowly  unwound,  a  movie.  Two  cans 
and  string  made  a  walkie-talkie.  Tutors  and  mothers  also  spray-painted 
cartons  for  use  in  storing  toys,  and  they  cut  down  large  soap-powder 
boxes  to  make  cases  for  childien's  books. 

Books  were  introduced  early  in  the  project,  though  the  attention  span 
of  these  15-month-olds  was  very  short.  To  win  the  children's  interest,  the 
tutors  tried  to  relate  things  seen  in  books  to  things  known  in  daily  life. 
Sometimes  they  would  carry  books  with  them  on  walks  and  point  to  a 
picture  of  a  dog  or  a  tree,  bird,  truck,  and  then  to  the  real  thing.  Adults 
and  children  in  illustrations  showing  family  groups  would  be  named  after 
the  persons  in  the  child's  family.  Books  that  a  child  found  especially 
appealing  would  be  presented  again  and  again,  unless  he  showed  boredom; 
books  that  had  no  attraction  for  him  would  be  taken  away  but  might  be 
introduced  again  later, 

One  boy  clearly  preferred  books  about  horses.  So  his  tutors  dug  up  as 
many  horse  books  as  they  could  find  and  they  took  him  on  trips  to  see 
horses  and  to  ride  ponies,  Whenever  a  tutor  showed  him  a  picture  of  a 
horse,  he  would  tell  her  about  these  trips.  Sometimes  he  would  sit  for  an 
hour  looking  at  a  book  with  pictures  of  horses  in  it.  A  boy  who  preferred 
books  about  animals  in  general  was  taken  not  only  to  the  zoo  several 
times,  but  also  to  the  Rock  Creek  Park  Nature  Center,  the  natural  history 
exhibits  at  the  Smithsonian,  and  the  circus.  And  his  mother  took  him  to 
the  country. 

With  the  child,  and  sometimes  his  mother,  watching  and  helping,  the 
tutors  made  books  by  cutting  pictures  from  magazines  and  pasting  them 
in  scrapbooks.  Some  of  the  books  were  concerned  with  a  single  subject 
-babies,  for  example,  or  automobiles,  Others  had  pictures  chosen  because 

37 


they  would  remind  the  child  of  things  he  and  the  tutoi  had  seen  at  the 
zoo,  or  in  a  stoie,  or  during  a  walk  around  the  block.  Scrapbooks  were 
also  made  to  illustrate  such  concepts  as  big  and  little  (an  elephant  and  a 
mouse),  one  and  many  (automobile  tires),  old  and  new  (shoes),  and  ciicle 
and  square.  Qthei  scrapbooks  were  used  to  teach  numbers.  A  picture  of 
one  sheep  would  be  headed,  "This  is  1 ";  of  two  donkeys,  "This  is  2,"  ami 
so  on.  The  numerals  were  cut  out  of  sandpaper  so  the  child  could  feel  as 
well  as  see  them.  The  homemade  books  were  left  in  the  home  for  use 
whenever  the  child  liked.  So  were  some  of  the  others,  particularly  the 
cloth  ones. 

The  tutors  also  told  stories,  read  stories,  and  used  the  combination  of 
story  book  and  record  player. 

During  their  third  year,  most  of  the  children  went  to  the  library  a 
number  of  times  and  checked  out,  carried  home,  and  kept  for  a  few  days  a 
book  of  their  own  choosing.  The  children  by  this  time  had  come  to  expucl 
the  presentation  of  at  least  one  book,  old  or  new,  dining  each  session,  mid 
books  often  took  up  more  of  the  session  than  toys.  "Hey,  Teach,*'  some 
of  the  children  would  say  as  the  tutor  entered,  "I  wanna  book." 

Music,  too,  was  part  of  the  curriculum.  The  tutors  would  sing  to  the 
children,  play  songs  and  other  music  on  a  record  player,  and  use  home- 
made or  inexpensive  children's  instruments— drum,  tambourine,  bells, 
xylophone— to  help  the  youngsters  express  rhythm  and  time. 

"When  the  babies  were  young  (15-18  months),"  reports  one  of  the 
tutois,  Patricia  Chernoff,  "I  held  them  in  my  lap  or  arms,  facing  me,  mid 
moved  my  body  or  knees  to  the  ihythm  of  the  song,  at  the  same  Lime 
articulating  the  words  carefully  and  diawing  the  child's  attention  to  my 
singing  by  holding  him  close  and  using  exaggerated  facial  expression,  I 
repeated  the  same  songs  until  eventually  the  child  attempted  to  sing.  At 
this  point,  I  simplified  the  words,  concentiating  on  those  which  were 
repeated  most  often  in  the  song  and  therefore  easiest  to  perceive  mid 
repeat.  For  example,  in  presenting  "Shoo,  Fly,  Don't  Bother  Me/'  1  sting 
Shoo  Fly  with  greater  emphasis  and  volume  than  the  rest  of  the  words, 
expecting  the  child  to  repeat  only  those  two  words.  Gradually,  when  he 
was  able  to  sing  Shoo  Fly  in  the  correct  places  throughout  the  song.  I 
encouraged  him  to  add  the  remainder  of  the  phrase,  don  't  bother  me.  The 
phrase,  for  I  belong  to  somebody,  because  of  its  length  and  the  rhythm 
with  which  it  is  sung,  comes  much  later.  When  the  child  is  unable  to 
perceive  the  words  from  the  recorded  presentation,  I  have  repeated  Ihoni 
more  slowly,  later  without  the  recording. 

"Once  the  child  has  become  interested  in  the  actual  singing  of  the 
songs,  I  have  lessened  body  contact  and  emphasized  the  rhythm,  con* 
centrating  only  on  the  words.  When  the  child  becomes  tired  of  singing,  I 
terminate  the  music  session  rather  than  changing  the  emphasis  to  clapping, 
etc. 

"I  have  found  that  the  length  of  time  required  to  learn  a  song  has 
lessened  considerably  (in  some  cases,  the  child  enters  in  during  the  first 
presentation)  as  the  children  become  able  to  focus  their  attention  on  tlic 

38 


words  and  as  I  continually  reinforce  with  praise  and  enthusiasm  their 
attempts  to  sing  the  words." 

A  child's  birthday  was  always  lecognized,  usually  by  a  party  and  a  book 
or  a  toy  from  the  tutor.  If  a  party  could  not  be  arranged,  tutor  and  child 
would  walk  to  a  store  to  pick  out  a  present. 


Influence  on  the  Mothers 

The  tutors  believe  that  they  have  reached  each  family  to  some  degree. 
One  mother  related  with  pride  that  whenever  she  took  her  boy  to  the 
store,  he  dragged  her  over  to  the  display  of  books,  and  she  sometimes 
bought  him  one.  Another  would  sit  in  on  the  tutoring  sessions  and  often 
ask  questions  about  aims  and  techniques.  Though  she  herself  could  not 
read,  she  took  out  a  library  card  at  the  tutors'  suggestion,  and  began 
boirowing  books  for  her  three  youngest. 

At  the  start,  one  mother  kept  screaming  at  her  15-month-old,  who  liked 
to  get  into  things:  "You  are  bad --I'm  going  to  beat  you."  Her  attitude  was 
hostile,  almost  rejecting.  As  she  watched  the  boy's  progress  under  the 
tutors,  though,  she  came  to  recognize  his  inquisitiveness  as  a  mark  of 
intelligence,  and  her  attitude  turned  to  one  of  acceptance  and  even  pride. 
Another  woman  frequently  made  out-of-town  visits,  leaving  her  boy  with 
a  relative.  During  these  absences  he  was  obviously  upset;  even  when  the 
mother  was  at  home  he  seemed  to  feel  insecure.  He  became  a  happy 
interested  youngster  only  after  the  tutors  persuaded  the  mother  that  he 
loved  and  needed  her,  and  she  began  staying  home.  Under  the  influence  of 
the  tutors,  A.'s  mother,  mentioned  earlier,  eased  her  harsh  discipline  and 
became  so  interested  in  learning  how  to  help  children  that  she  took  a 
volunteer  job  in  a  nursery  school.  A.'s  IQ,  which  had  dropped  from  1 1 6  at 
21  months  to  89  at  36  months,  rose  to  102  at  4  yeais. 

Some  of  the  women  would  buy  a  toy  or  a  book  like  one  brought  in  by 
the  tutor,  or  they  would  ask  what  they  ought  to  buy.  One  displayed  a 
28-piece  puzzle  she  had  bought  as  a  Christmas  present.  Since  the  boy  in 
the  program  was  only  two  and  a  half,  the  tutor  supposed  it  was  for  the 
older  children.  "No,  it's  for  him, "  said  the  mother.  She  dumped  the  pieces 
on  the  floor,  and  the  youngster  put  them  together  in  a  few  minutes.  The 
boy's  older  sister  found  it  difficult  at  times  to  keep  up  with  him.  The 
mother  called  her  silly  or  dumb,  till  the  tutor  explained  that  the  girl  was 
smart  enough-she  just  had  not  had  her  brother's  training. 

A  number  of  the  families  began  taking  more  trips  with  their  children. 
Some  mothers  had  not  known  that  such  resources  as  libraries,  museums, 
and  a  zoo  existed;  others  had  not  known  how  to  get  to  them.  A  few 
mothers  hadn't  even  known  what  bus  to  take  to  go  downtown,  and  one 
nevei  left  the  house  to  go  anywhere  unless  one  of  the  children  went  along: 
she  didn't  think  she  could  find  her  way  back. 

The  tutors  often  encountered  problems  that  were  outside  their  province 
as  tutors  but  not  as  human  beings.  For  instance,  they  would  find  a  family 
sitting  around  in  winter  coats  because  the  heat  had  been  turned  off.  Or 

39 


the  mother  would  be  distraught  because  the  family  faced  eviction.  Or  t 
mother  and  father  had  quarrelled,  and  the  mother  had  gone  off  to  vi 
relatives.  The  children  in  such  cases  would  be  upset-fearful,  or  apathct 
or  quarrelsome;  one  child  kept  asking  his  tutor  to  take  him  with  her. 

One  woman  with  half  a  dozen  children  had  an  especially  difficult  tii 
with  her  money.  (Says  a  tutor:  "To  budget  welfare  money-which  1st 
low  anyway-for  a  month  is  really  a  challenge  even  to  someone  who  1 
taken  a  home  economics  couise  in  college.  When  the  welfare  checks  coi 
out,  food  stores  raise  their  prices,  and  people  go  around  in  cars  trying 
get  you  to  buy  nice  looking  but  flimsy  clothes  that  won't  last  ovei 
month.")  After  the  fast  two  or  three  weeks  of  the  month,  this  worn 
would  run  out  of  both  food  and  money.  Several  times  a  tutor  found  t 
children  picking  crumbs  from  the  floor  and  eating  them.  The  project  h 
no  funds  to  feed  the  family,  but  the  tutors  sent  the  mother  to  agcnc 
that  could  give  her  emergency  help.  They  also  went  shopping  with  her  a 
showed  her  how  to  choose  economical  foods,  and  they  brought  in  snac 
for  the  children. 

When  the  children  were  three,  the  tutoring  ended,  except  for  twic< 
month  visits,  and  the  project  tried  to  place  them  in  nursery  schools. 
succeeded  in  all  but  two  cases,  where  the  family  situation  was  particula 
unstable.  (An  effort  to  place  children  from  the  control  group  succeeded 
only  half  a  dozen  cases,  partly  because  the  effort  was  less  intensive  a 
partly  because  the  families  were  little  motivated  to  send  their  children 
nursery  school.) 

At  the  same  time  the  mothers  were  invited  to  meet  once  a  month* 
groups  of  four  or  five,  for  a  planned  program  of  discussions  of  subjects 
which  they  were  interested  as  well  as  subjects  about  which  the  proj< 
thought  they  needed  to  be  informed.  Most  were  pleased  to  accept  and  o 
was  delighted:  she  had  always  wanted,  she  said,  to  belong  to  something. 
All  kinds  of  problems  have  been  discussed-for  example,  how  to  t 
older  children  about  sex  (some  of  the  women  said  that  their  own  moth 
had  been  too  shy  to  talk  to  them,  so  they  hadn't  known  about  havinj 
baby  until  they'd  had  it);  what  to  do  about  a  child  who  is  having  troul 
in  school  or  refuses  to  go  to  school;  how  to  cope  with  poor  health,  t 
housing,  marital  problems.  The  tutors  conducting  the  meetings  have  f 
equipped  to  answer  some  of  the  questions  themselves.  They  have  hand 
the  others  by  giving  out  pamphlets  on  the  subject  or  suggesting  that  1 
mother  talk  with  her  child's  counselor  at  the  school.  Movies  have  be 
differences  between  children  at  different  ages,  about  sibl 


others  have  gone  back  to  school  or  have  entered  training  programs,  in 
order  to  get  better  jobs.  Some  have  sent  their  children  off  to  school  or  a 
day  care  center,  traveled  an  hour  or  an  hour  and  a  half  to  attend  a  training 
program  across  town,  and  hurried  home  late  in  the  afternoon.  After  the 
children  entered  nursery  school,  several  of  the  mothers  coupled  an  all-day 
job  with  a  training  program  at  night.  They  would  leave  their  children  at  a 
sitter's  home  five  days  a  week,  picking  them  up  on  Friday  night  and 
retaining  them  on  Sunday. 

During  the  kindergarten  year,  the  homes  of  working  mothers  will  be 
visited  on  Saturdays,  and  the  children  of  these  mothers  will  be  seen  at  the 
babysitter's  during  the  week. 


Implications  for  Educational  Policies 

To  summarize,  the  Infant  Education  Research  Project  found  that: 

1 .  A  tutoring  program  beginning  at  the  age  of  1 5  months  for  Negro 
boys  from  disadvantaged  families  and  continuing,  1  hour  a  day,  for  21 
months  significantly  raised  their  level  of  intelligence  until  they  were  4 
years  old,  at  least, 

2.  The  average  reached  at  the  end  of  the  third  year,  106,  dropped  six 
points  during  the  next  year  but  was  still  significantly  higher  than  the 
control  group's  average, 

3.  When  maternal  interest  was  high,  a  child's  IQ  was  likely  also  to  be 
high. 

How  long  the  benefits  from  the  additional  stimulation  will  continue 
remains  to  be  seen.'  But  the  drop  in  the  peak  IQ  level  after  the  tutoring 
stopped,  at  3,  suggests  to  Schaefer  that  short-term  programs  of  early 
education  are  not  sufficient  to  develop  and  sustain  the  child's  potential 
over  the  long  run.  Other  studies  using  different  programs  of  stimulation 
point  to  the  same  conclusion. 

The  investigator  thinks  the  answer  lies  in  both  early  and  continued 
education.  "Genetics  may  determine  the  potential  range  of  a  child's  intel- 
lectual level,"  he  points  out,  "but  the  quality  of  the  environment  deter- 
mines the  actual  level.  Evidence  is  accumulating  rapidly  that  because  of 
physical,  social  and  emotional,  and  cultural  deprivations,  many  children 
are  not  developing  their  genetic  potential,  and  therefore  do  not  function 
effectively  in  school  and  in  society." 

Physical  deprivations  include  inadequate  medical  care,  insufficient  and 
low-quality  food,  poor  'housing,  and  inadequate  clothing.  Social  and 
emotional  deprivations  stem  from  the  lack  of  stable  and  supporting 
relationships  with  the  mother  and  father  or  their  substitutes.  Cultural 
deprivation  occurs  when  parents  fail  to  provide  a  stimulating  environment. 
Children  who  lack  pencils,  paper,  crayons,  books,  games,  and  other  educa- 
tional materials  do  not  receive  enough  training  in  certain  qualities  and 
abilities -such  as  attention,  concentration,  perseverence,  and  perceptual- 
motor  skills-that  make  for  success  in  school.  Children  who  are  not  en- 
couraged to  talk  about  their  experiences,  who  do  not  have  models  of  good 

41 


language  use  to  follow  in  their  early  yeais,  and  who  have  little  01 
opportunity  to  make  visits  beyond  their  immediate  neighborhood 
likely  to  reach  school  age  lacking  the  language  skills,  the  interest,  and 
knowledge  of  more  fortunate  children. 

As  Schacfer  sees  it,  the  intellectual  development  of  the  typical  child 
be  viewed  as  having  four  basic  stages.  In  the  first,  the  paient  devclo 
loving  acceptance  of  the  child  and  a  positive  involvement  with  him.  In 
second,  this  involvement  elicits  from  the  child  the  development  \ 
positive  relationship  with  the  parent.  In  the  third,  the  parent  and  the  c 
engage  together  in  activities,  such  as  piling  up  blocks,  rolling  a  ball,  li 
ing  at  a  picture  book,  and  the  parent  by  word  and  by  example  teaches 
child  language,  skills,  and  task-oriented  behavior.  From  this  early 
perience  with  the  parent,  the  child  reaches  the  fourth  basic  stage- he 
acquired  the  interests  and  the  skills  that  enable  him  to  learn  on  hist 
"Successful  achievement  of  these  early  developmental  stages  in 
home,"  says  Schaefer,  "may  be  a  necessary  basis  for  a  successful  ccl 
tion  in  the  school.  But  deprivation  during  their  own  childhood  may  I 
parents  without  the  personal  resources  to  support  the  optimal  devi 
ment  of  their  children." 

Do  we  then  need  more  nursery  school?  More  child-care  centers  thai 
take  children  at  15  months  and  even  younger?  A  spread  of  tutoring 
grams,  like  the  one  in  Washington  but  lasting  longer  and  made  part  ol 
public  education  system  so  that  every  child  will  be  reached? 

"We  do  need  to  recognize  that  education  goes  on  from  birth,"  Scha 
answers,  "but  I  think  it  would  be  more  fruitful  in  most  instance 
support  parents  in  their  educational  role  than  to  set  up  educational  i 
tutions  to  supplant  them.  Parents,  or  at  least  the  mother,  are  there  t 
and  continue  to  be  there.  This  study  found  that  both  tutoring  and 
quality  of  maternal  care  were  related  to  the  children's  intellectual  dcvi 
ment  at  three  and  four.  Tutoring  can  be  thought  of  as  suppleraon 
maternal,  or  parental,  care  because  in  the  middle  class  a  good  mothci, 
a  good  father,  does  what  the  tutois  have  done." 

As  one  hopeful  development,  Schaefer  points  to   a  new   Insli1 
supported  project  in  Prince  Georges  County,  Maryland,  that  seeks  lo 
grate  the  educational  efforts  of  home  and  school.  When  children  ur 
fants,  teachers  will  go  into  the  home  to  work  with  the  parents;  late 
the  parents  will  go  into  the  school.  But  he  thinks  we  ought  to  look 
farther  ahead.  "If  you  assume  that  almost  everyone  becomes  a  parent 
that  one  of  the  most  important  jobs  for  each  generation  is  to  rear  the 
generation,"  he  says,  "it  follows  that  we  should  be  giving  children,  to 
ning  in  kindergarten  and  running  through  the  twelfth  grade,  some  o 
skills  needed  if  they  are  to  be  competent  as  parents.  We  should 
programs  for  future  parents." 

The  Washington  project  has  demonstrated  that  lower-class  black  in 
can  benefit  from  additional  stimulation.  Could  other  infants  also  ben 
Schaefer  answers  that  we  cannot  be  sure  until  programs  of  stimulatio 
tested  with  other  ethnic  and  socioeconomic  groups.  "Some  peopU 
that  middle-class  parents  are  doing  as  well  as  they  can  with  their  chil 

42 


—that  nothing  else  they  could  do  would  lead  to  higher  levels  of  intelli- 
gence and  achievement.  I  don't  believe  that,  but  it  needs  to  be  tested." 

"Many  parents— and  I  think  they  can  be  found  in  all  social  classes- 
don't  have  the  skills  to  be  effective  in  their  roles  as  teachers.  We  need  to 
develop  methods  of  improving  the  education  of  young  children,  and  we 
also  need  to  develop  better  ways  of  communicating  what  we  learn— and 
what  we  already  know,  for  that  matter-to  all  parents  and  future  parents. 
If  the  whole  culture  became  aware  of  the  importance  of  parents  as 
teachers,  I  believe  it  would  lead  to  an  educational  revolution,  and  to  a 
bettei  adjusted,  more  competent,  and  more  intelligent  population." 

Research  Grant.  MH  9224 

Dates  of  Interviews.  August  and  September  1969 

References: 

Bayley,  N.,  and  Schaefer,  E.  S., Correlations  of  maternal  and  child  behaviors  with  the 
development  of  mental  abilities:  data  from  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study.  Mono- 
graphs of  the  Society  for  Research  in  Child  Development,  29:6,  1964. 

Schaefer,  E.  S.  Home  tutoring  program.  Children,  16:2,  1969. 

Skeels,  H.  M.  Adult  status  of  children  with  contrasting  early  life  experiences.  Mono- 
graphs of  the  Society  for  Research  in  Child  Development,  31:3,  1966. 

U.S.  Office  of  Education,  Department  of  Health,  Education,  and  Welfare.  Infant 
Education  Research  Project.  Washington,  D.C.,:  Superintendent  of  Documents, 
U.S.  Government  Printing  Office. 

U.S.  Department  of  Health,  Education,  and  Welfare.  PHS  Health  Services  and  Mental 
Health  Administration.  The  edge  of  education.  HSMHA  World,  4(1):  18-23, 
Jan-Feb  1969. 


43 


Director. 

Constance  N.  Swander 

Co  Director- 
Gladys  R,  Blankenship 
The  Good  Samaritan  Center 
San  Antonio,  Texas 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Introduction  and  Background 

Carmen,  David,  Pablo,  Maria,  and  the  dozen  other  3-year-olds  playfo; 
the  coxirtyard  of  the  Good  Samaritan  Center  in  San  Antonio  strike 
observer  as  happy,  energetic,  and  bright.  And  they  are.  However,  wer 
not  for  a  special  educational  program  developed  and  tested  by  the  Con 
with  financial  support  from  NIMH,  many  of  them  would  be  destined  i 
to  enter  the  public  school  system  so  poorly  prepared  as  to  be  consiile 
and  treated  as  slow-a  few  of  them,  possibly,  as  retarded.  Typically,  tl 
course  through  the  school  system  would  grow  increasingly  difficult,  im 
number  would  drop  out  early,  prepared  only  for  living  at  the  I 
socioeconomic  and  cultural  level  into  which  they  were  born. 

The  Good  Samaritan  Center  is  a  neighborhood  center  serving  abi 
20,000  people  on  San  Antonio's  west  side.  This  is  a  low-income  ar 
more  than  90  percent  of  whose  residents  are  of  Mexican  descent.  Thoi 
the  neighborhood  is  clearly  poor,  it  is  not  a  slum.  About  650  families  1 
in  two  public  housing  projects;  the  others  in  small,  one-story  house 
some  of  them  little  better  than  shacks-covering  block  after  block.  Mi 
of  the  houses,  which  would  sell  for  perhaps  $6,000,  are  owned  by  I 
families  living  in  them  or  by  a  relative.  They  are  usually  ncnt,  I 
crowded;  living  rooms  must  often  double  as  bedrooms. 

In  the  four  census  tracts  surrounding  the  Center,  the  median  nnm 
family  income  in  1960  ranged  from  $2,830  to  $4,190.  More  than 
percent  of  the  families  being  served  in  the  Good  Samaritan's  health  a 
guidance  clinics  had  monthly  incomes  of  less  than  $200  to  support  froiv 
to  1 5  persons.  Among  the  families  of  children  now  being  served  by  t 
pre-school  project,  about  a  third  are  on  relief.  Fathers  are  absent  in  abo 
a  fifth  of  the  homes.  The  men  typically  are  unskilled  or  semi-skill' 
workers;  the  women  typically  do  not  work  outside  the  home. 

Good  Samaritan,  located  in  the  midst  of  the  area  it  serves,  is  housed 
half  a  dozen  simply  designed,  one-story  structures  built  around  a  com 
yard.  It  is  sponsored  by  the  West  Texas  Diocese  of  the  Episcopal  Chun 

44 


and  is  supported  by  the  church  and  by  the  United  Fund  of  San  Antonio 
and  Bexar  County. 

Studying  the  problems  common  to  the  people  of  its  neighboihood,  the 
Center  found  them  to  be  rooted  in  lack  of  education,  which  in  turn  was 
rooted  in  lack  of  preparation  for  school.  Sixty  percent  of  the  children  in 
first  grade  were  considered  problem  children;  15  percent  of  all  the 
children  had  been  held  back  at  least  one  year.  Many  of  the  teen-age 
dropouts  could  read  and  write  English  only  haltingly;  they  could  not 
express  themselves  adequately  even  in  their  first  language,  Spanish.  Half  of 
the  adults  in  the  neighborhood  had  not  completed  fifth  grade. 

Constance  N.  Swandei,  executive  director  of  the  Center,  decided  in 
1964  that  the  long-run  solution  to  the  area's  poverty  and  associated 
problems  lay  in  preparing  the  children  for  success  in  school.  She  planned 
to  do  this  through  a  pre-school  program  that  would  teach  English  while 
pieserving  and  reinforcing  the  children's  use  of  Spanish,  and  at  the  same 
time  would  develop  the  children's  ability  to  learn  by  guiding  them 
through  planned  learning  experiences. 

The  program,  which  won  NIMH  support  and  opened  in  1965,  had  those 
two  principal  objectives  because  Mrs.  Swander  recognized  that  the 
children  had  two  principal  handicaps-lack  of  an  opportunity  to  ,learn 
English  and  also  lack  of  the  verbal  stimulation  necessary  for  a  child  to  de- 
velop whatever  intellectual  capacity  he  was  born  with.  In  the  typical  dis- 
advantaged  home  of  San  Antonio's  west  side,  as  in  many  disadvantaged 
homes  elsewhere,  children  are  likely  to  be  ignored  unless  they  misbehave, 
and  language  is  used  more  for  controlling  their  behavior  than  for  telling 
them  about  objects  in  their  environment  or  for  otherwise  instructing 
them.  So  they  grow  up  lacking  real  facility  even  in  their  native  language. 
And  without  language  facility,  points  out  Shari  Nedler,  until  recently  the 
project's  psychologist,  "the  child  cannot  organize  his  concepts,  he  cannot 
reason  at  abstract  levels,  he  cannot  describe,  analyze  or  synthesize;  he 
cannot  solve  any  but  the  simplest  problems." 

The  1 6  children  who  enter  the  program  each  year  are  chosen  at  random 
from  the  neighborhood's  eligible  3-year-olds,  who  are  found  by  a 
house-to-house  canvass.  To  be  eligible,  a  child  must  come  from  a  low- 
income,*  Spanish-speaking  family  that  has  lived  at  least  five  years  in  the 
city  and  two  years  in  the  neighborhood.  This  residential  requirement 
makes  for  a  stable  sample;  in  four  years  the  project  has  lost  only  two 
children.  Until  the  1 969-70  class  of  3-year-olds  was  chosen,  there  was  also 
a  requirement  that  the  child  be  able  to  speak  only  in  Spanish.  This 
requirement  has  been  dropped,  partly  because  most  of  the  neighborhood's 
children  do  know  a  little  English,  picked  up  from  television  and  from 
brothers  and  sisters  who  go  to  school,  and  partly  because  the  Center 
wishes  to  emphasize  that  the  program  is  potentially  valuable  for  any 


*The  Orshansky  poverty  index  is  used.  A  family  is  considered  in  the  low-income 
group  if,  for  example,  it  numbers  seven  persons  and  has  an  annual  income  of  $4,700  or 
less. 

45 


culturally  disadvantaged  child,  whatever  the  language  he  hears  at  home. 
When  the  eligible  children-there  were  54  of  them  out  of  the  130-3-year- 
olds  found  in  the  most  recent  canvass-have  been  identified,  a  table  of 
random  numbers  is  used  to  choose  the  16  whom  the  program  can  accept, 
During  the  first  two  years,  the  staff  had  some  difficulty  persuading 
families  to  let  their  children  participate.  Several  families,  indeed,  flatty 
lefused.  Others  had  to  be  visited  as  many  as  half  a  dozen  times  by  tlte 
Center's  principal  emissary,  Gladys  R.  Blankenship,  who  is  of  Spanish 
descent   herself   and   thoroughly  bilingual.  Mrs.   Blankenship  is  super- 
intendent of  the  school  and  co-director  of  the  project  with  Mrs.  Swamter. 
The  families  knew  and  ti  listed  the  Good  Samaritan  Center;  many  of  them 
just  did  not  think  a  child  of  3  was  old  enough  to  be  parted  from  Iris 
mother,  even   if  she  did  have-as  she  generally  did-toc  many  other 
children  to  pay  him  much  attention.  Today,  motheis  throughout  Hie 
neighborhood  are  eager  to  have  their  children  accepted  in  the  program. 
And  "Los  Ninos,"  a  weekly  television  show  starring  Mis.  Blankenship  and 
several  children  from  the  Center,  undertaken  in  1969  at  the  request  of 
Station  KENS,  is  popular  in  San  Antonio  and  neighboring  communities. 
In  1968,  with  the  grant  from  NIMH  due  to  expire  in  two  years,  (lie 
Office  of  Education  made  the  project  an  arm  of  the  Southwest  Educa- 
tional Development  Laboratory,  in  Austin,  Texas,  one  of  the  Office's  1 6 
regional  laboratories,  and  the  laboratory  named  the  project's  psychologist, 
Mrs.  Nedler,  as  its  program  director  for  early  childhood  education.  Known 
now  as  the  San  Antonio  Urban  Educational  Development  Center,  the 
project  is  serving  as  a  model  for  other  efforts  with  pre-school  children  from 
disadvantaged  groups.  As  of  early  1970,  programs  to  further  test  the 
methods  and   curricula  developed   under  the  NIMH  grant  were  going 

forward : 

-in  San  Antonio  with  400  Mexican-American  children,  a  program 
conducted  with  Model  City  funding  in  the  public  schools; 

-In  McAllen,  Texas,  with  150  children  of  migrant  Mexican- American 
farm  workers; 

-in  Dallas,  Texas,  with  3-year-old  Negro  children  in  a  recently 
established  school  for  disadvantaged  children; 

-in  Bossier  City,  Louisiana,  with  50  Negro  children. 
Children  from  disadvantaged  Negro  families,  Mrs.  Nedler  points  out, 
have  basically  the  same  problems  as  those  from  disadvantaged  Mexican- 
American  families:  they  are  not  learning  good  English  at  home,  and  they 
are  not  getting  the  intellectual  stimulation  necessary  for  the  development 
of  language  skills  and  cognitive  abilities. 

School  sessions  at  the  San  Antonio  center  run  for  three  hours  each 
morning,  five  days  a  week.  Each  class  of  16  has  one  young  woman  as 
teacher  and  another  as  assistant  teacher.  The  teachers  are  college  women 
who  have  majored  in  education  or  in  child  development  and  who  speak 
Spanish  fluently.  The  assistants  are  high  school  graduates  from  the  same 
neighborhood  as  the  children.  Trained  on  the  job,  they  serve  not  so  much 
as  aides-if  this  term  connotes  someone  who  helps  with  coats,  serves 
snacks,  carries  messages-hut  as  second  teachers, 

46 


Building  Confidence 

Childien  in  the  Good  Samaritan  area,  like  children  in  other  disadvan- 
taged  neighborhoods  (and  some  children  in  more  favored  neighborhoods), 
typically  receive  little  encouragement  to  begin  a  task  and  can  have  small 
hope  of  a  reward  for  completing  it.  As  long  as  a  child  is  doing  what  he's 
supposed  to  do,  his  parents  say  little  or  nothing  to  him;  they  wouldn't 
think  of  praising  him.  But  let  him  do  what  he  is  not  supposed  to  do,  and 
he  is  reprimanded.  Such  conditions  stifle  initiative  and  promote  passivity. 
They  may  promote  misbehavior  as  well,  because  misbehavior  becomes  one 
way  of  getting  attention. 

Through  all  its  many  activities,  the  school  tries  to  develop  a  child's 
confidence  and  wholesome  self-regard.  At  the  very  start,  for  example, 
each  child  is  introduced  to  the  class  and  given  a  name  tag,  which  he  wears 
proudly.  Also,  he  is  taken  on  a  tour  of  the  classroom  and  shown  where 
things  are  kept,  how  to  put  them  away,  how  to  care  for  books,  and  how 
to  carry  a  chair.  Putting  this  information  to  use  not  only  promotes  good 
classroom  discipline  but  also  gives  the  child  a  feeling  of  accomplishment. 

Classroom  performance  checklists,  along  with  other  observations,  are 
used  to  assign  the  children  to  three  smaller  groups  based  on  ability  levels. 
Such  grouping  gives  each  child  opportunities  to  experience  success  in 
work  geared  to  his  ability  01  readiness.  The  groupings  vary  for  diffeient 
activities  and  are  adjusted  throughout  the  year.  The  teacher  leads  one 
group  and  the  assistant  another;  members  of  the  third  group  engage  in 
individual  projects-painting,  building,  cutting  out  designs,  and  so  on. 

Another  aid  to  building  self-confidence  and  a  feeling  of  identity  is  "tell 
time,"  a  penod  right  after  roll-call  during  which  a  child  may  come  before 
the  class  and  say  anything  he  wants  to  say.  The  children  don't  have  to 
participate,  but  most  of  them  usually  do.  They  talk  about  their  mother  or 
father,  or  something  that  happened  at  home,  01  a  cut  finger-anything. 
Even  the  3-year-olds  are  eager  to  express  themselves. 

Cliildren  are  praised  when  they  answer  correctly.  They  are  also  praised 
for  working  hard  and  behaving  appropriately.  A  child  who  is  unusually 
shy  or  passive  is  given  special  attention. 


Teaching  English 

In  the  beginning,  the  Center  thought  that  the  children  would  learn 
English  much  as  they  had  learned  Spanish,  by  being  exposed  to  it.  As  they 
took  part  in  nursery  school  activities,  the  teacher  would  talk  to  them  in 
English,  using  Spanish  to  explain.  They  would  repeat  her  English  words 
and  gradually  begin  to  use  them:  they  would  "pick  up"  the  new  language. 
Tests  at  the  end  of  the  year,  however,  showed  that  while  the  children  had 
made  progress  on  other  fronts,  they  had  advanced  hardly  at  all  in  their 
knowledge  of  English. 

The  school  then  began  developing  a  program  for  teaching  English  sys- 
tematically. Based  on  its  first-year  experience  and  on  word  lists  used  in 

47 


the  public  schools,  it  drew  up  a  2,800-word  vocabulary  and  organized 
by  topical  units-words  dealing  with  the  body,  with  food,  with  clothin 
with  transportation,  and  so  on.  Vocabulary  building,  however,  is  not* 
end  in  itself.  The  school's  aim  is  to  develop  a  child's  competence  in  h 
first  language  by  expanding  his  basic  fund  of  information  through  ne 
labels,  or  words,  and  new  concepts,  or  ideas,  and  then  to  introduce  him  1 
English  by  using  the  same  labels  and  concepts. 

During  the  first  15  weeks  of  school  the  3-year  olds  hear  nothing  bi 
Spanish,  for  these  children  typically  have  a  poor  vocabulary  even  in  the 
own  language.  As  their  teacher  expresses  it,  they  lack  labels.  The 
mothers,  unlike  most  middle-class  mothers,  have  not  been  inclined  t 
name  things— and  thus  to  teach  the  names— when  talking  to  their  childrci 
They  have  been  saying,  "Get  me  that,"  instead  of  "Get  me  the  apple, 
and  "Put  it  here,"  instead  of  "Put  the  apple  on  the  table." 

Lessons  on  body  awareness,  which  is  the  first  topical  unit,  begin  wit 
the  use  of  a  mirror  to  aid  in  self-identification,  a  pre-requisite  of  sel 
esteem.  Many  of  the  children  have  not  used  a  mirror  to  any  extent— sonn 
not  at  all— and  are  not  fully  aware  of  their  own  features,  The  teacher  ask; 
in  Spanish,  ""Whom  do  you  see?"  Generally  the  child  responds,  "Me,"S 
the  teacher  asks,  "Well,  what  is  your  name?"  Then  she  asks  everyone  t 
say,  "Good  morning,  Juan." 

After  other  members  of  the  group  have  been  similaily  introduced,  labe 
ing  begins.  The  teacher  points  to  her  nose,  eyes,  mouth  and  names  their 
then  she  has  the  children,  one  by  one,  say  nose,  eyes,  mouth— in  Spanish- 
and  point  to  these  parts  on  their  own  faces.  Next  the  teacher  touches 
child's  arm.  "Este  es  el  brazo,"  she  says.  "Este  es  el  brazo,  verdad?1 
Touching  his  ears,  she  says,  "Y,  estas  son  sus  orejos— orejos,  si."  When  sh 
asks  a  child  where  his  ears  are,  he  may  hesitate.  At  home,  undoubtedly  h< 
has  heard  the  word  orejos,  but  he  has  not  quite  connected  it  with  his  owi 
orejos.  The  teacher  comes  back  to  him  every  once  in  a  while  until  it  i 
clear  that  the  connection  has  been  made. 

As  part  of  the  body  awareness  unit,  the  class  makes  a  life-size  piizzto 
The  teacher  traces  around  a  child;  then  asks,  "What  is  missing-what  doe 
he  need  to  be  able  to  see?  To  be  able  to  hear?"  and  so  on.  Once  tin 
missing  features  are  put  in,  the  teacher  cuts  the  figure  into  parts,  and  cad 
child  gets  the  opportunity  to  put  it  together.  He  is  encouraged  to  tiill 
about  what  he  is  doing. 

Games  such  as  Simon  Says  are  also  part  of  the  curriculum.  As  witl 
every  new  activity,  the  teacher  and  the  assistant  teacher  first  demonstraK 
how  the  game  is  played.  Then  they  lead  groups  in  playing  it.  Eventually 
as  a  means  of  building  self-esteem,  each  child  takes  his  turn  at  being  tin 
leader. 

At  the  conclusion  of  a  unit,  a  performance  checklist  is  administered,  li 
the  case  of  body  awareness,  the  teachers  ask,  "Can  this  child  identify 
himself  by  name?  Label  parts  of  the  body?  Locate  them  on  a  doll  and  or 
himself?  Describe  simple  functions?"  Lagging  children  receive  special  at 
tention. 

48 


From  body  awareness,  the  curriculum  proceeds  to  clothing.  The  teacher 
introduces  the  unit  by  giving  the  rule  for  this  class  of  objects:  "If  you  can 
wear  it,  it's  clothing."  She  then  labels  articles  of  clothing.  "Who  has  a 
dress  on?"  she  asks.  "I  have,"  several  girls  respond.  Says  the  teacher: 
"How  do  we  know  the  dress  is  clothing?  Because  we  can  wear  it."  After 
she  has  labeled  several  pieces  of  clothing,  she  points  to  a  chair  and  asks, 
"Is  this  clothing?"  "Why  not?"  "Can  we  wear  it?" 

The  clothing  lessons  introduce  the  children  to  the  concept  of  fasteners. 
"Can  you  find  buttons  on  your  dress?"  asks  the  teacher.  "What  do  you 
suppose  would  happen  if  you  didn't  have  buttons  on  your  dress?"  "What 
other  ways  can  you  fasten  clothing?" 

Sometimes  a  child  will  point  to  a  zippei  and  name  it  in  English.  "Yes," 
the  teacher  will  say,  "that's  very  good.  That's  the  way  it  is  in  English- 
zipper  Now,  do  you  know  how  we  say  it  in  Spanish?"  They  want  to 
know,  so  she  teaches  them:  segadura. 

The  children  are  also  taught  the  labels  of  such  things  in  the  house  as 
stove,  sink,  chah,  and  table.  The  3-year-olds  have  a  model  kitchen  where 
they  look  at  and  touch  the  objects  they  are  labeling. 

In  many  learning  situations,  action  is  required  because  it  strengthens 
the  learning  process.  Half  a  dozen  4-year-olds,  for  example,  gather  with 
their  teacher  in  a  screened-off  corner  of  the  classroom  for  an  English 
lesson.  "We  are  standing,"  the  teacher  says,  and  places  each  child  on  his 
feet.  "We  are  standing,"  the  teacher  repeats.  "We  are  standing,"  say  the 
children.  The  teacher  begins  jumping  up  and  down,  and  the  group  follows. 
"We  are  jumping,"  the  teacher  says.  "We  are  jumping,"  say  the  children. 
"That's  right,"  says  the  teacher.  "We  are  jumping.  Now  say  it  again:  'We 
are  jumping.'  "  "We  are  jumping,"  say  the  children.  The  teacher  sits  down, 
and  the  group  follows,  "We  are  sitting,"  she  says.  "We  are  sitting,"  the 
class  repeats.  "Good,"  says  the  teacher,  "very  good." 

All  age  groups  study  the  same  topical  units-food,  clothing,  house,  and 
so  on-but  the  older  the  children,  the  more  deeply  each  subject  is  ex- 
plored. In  the  case  of  fasteners,  for  instance,  the  3-year-olds  learn  the 
labels  for  some  types  of  them,  and  the  4-year  olds  expand  this  vocabulary. 
The  5-year-olds  are  led  to  consider  the  purpose  of  fasteners.  The  teacher 
holds  up  a  child's  shirt  and  asks  first  David  and  then  Maria  to  try  it  on, 
They  can't  get  into  it,  though,  because  the  front  has  been  sewn  up.  "What 
can  we  do  about  this?"  the  teacher  asks.  "How  can  we  make  this  shirt 
easier  to  put  on?" 

"Cut  it,"  one  child  suggests. 

"Tear  it  open,"  says  another. 

"Fine,"  the  teacher  says,  and  proceeds  to  rip  the  shirt  down  the  front. 

But  now  when  the  children  try  it  on,  it  doesn't  function  as  a  shirt. 
Again  the  teacher  asks  for  suggestions.  "What  can  we  do  now?  How  can 
we  make  this  shirt  stay  together?" 

"A  button,"  somebody  ventures. 

Somebody  else  says:  "A  zipper." 

49 


"Good,1*  says  the  teacher.  "Let's  see  what  a  button  will  do."  She  s 
one  on.  The  group  talks  about  what  a  button  does  and  then  generalize 
othei  kinds  of  fasteners. 

Lessons  are  based  on  what  the  child  aheady  knows.  When  English 
sons  begin  for  the  3-year-olds  in  January,  the  introduction  of  words  a 
concepts  in  English  is  preceded  by  a  short  review  of  the  same  words  fi 
concepts  in  his  first  language.  For  instance,  starting  a  unit  dealing  w 
vehicles,  teacher  and  children  talk  about  un  carro,  tin  aemplano,  and 
bus.  Then  the  teacher  announces  that  everyone  will  speak  in  English. 

Teacher,  holding  up  a  picture  of  an  automobile:  "This  is  a  car." 

Childien;  car. 

T:  All  right.  Say  it,  "This  is  a  car." 

C:  This  is  a  car. 

T:  All  right.  This  is  a  car.  Good. 

T:  holding  up  a  picture  of  an  airplane:  This  is  an  airplane. 

C:  Airplane. 

T:  Good.  Again- 

C;  This  is  an  airplane. 

T:  All  right,  Martin,  say  "airplane." 

Boy:  Ah  plane. 

At  each  age  level  the  teacher  listens  to  each  child  in  the  group  as  he  ti 
to  reproduce  a  new  word. 

Children,  as  teacher  displays  a  picture  of  a  bus:  "This  is  a  bus." 

Teacher:  A  bus.  Again- 

C:  This  is  a  bus. 

T:  All  right.  Let's  have  Martin  say  it. 

Boy:  Bus. 

T:  Bus.  Good.  Now  let's  say  the  whole  thing. 

C:  This  is  a  bus. 

T:  Very  good, 

Then  the  teacher  asks  questions  requiring  a  "yes"  or  "no"  answer  (' 
this  a  car?")  and  other  questions  testing  whether  or  not  the  child  li 
related  the  woid  to  the  picture  ("All  right,  Martin,  show  me  the  a 
plane"). 

The  next  year,  when  the  children  are  4,  the  language  lesson  is  entire 
in  English. 

Teacher  (displaying  pictures  of  an  airplane,  a  truck,  and  a  ship):  "The 
are  vehicles." 

Children:  Vehicles.  These  are  vehicles. 

T:  Good.  Why  are  these  vehicles? 

Several  children:  Because  they  have  motors. 

T:  Because  they  have  motors  and  because  we  can  ride  in  them. 

T:  (showing  airplane):  Do  you  ride  in  this? 

C:  Yes. 

T:  Then  it  is  a  vehicle.  Say  it. 

C:  Then  it  is  a  vehicle. 

T:  (showing  ship):  Can  you  ride  in  this? 

C:  Yes, 

50 


T:  Say  it.  Then  it  is  a  vehicle. 

C:  Then  it  is  a  vehicle. 

T:  Listen,  if  you  can  ride  in  it,  it's  a  vehicle. 

T:  (with  picture  of  a  shirt):  Can  you  ride  in  this? 

C:No. 

T:So? 

C:  Not  a  vehicle. 

T:  It's  not  a  vehicle.  That's  veiy  good. 

T:  This  vehicle  is  a  truck. 

C:  This  vehicle  is  a  tiuck. 

T:  Good  for  you.  This  vehicle  is  a  truck.  Cynthia? 

Girl:  This  vehicle  is  a  truck. 

T:  Good. 

The  teacher  varies  the  pace  of  presentation  in  order  to  keep  the  chil- 
dren involved.  They  must  listen  carefully  if  they  are  to  answer  correctly: 

T:  Is  this  a  vehicle:  Is  this  a  ship? 

C:  Yes. 

T:  Okay.  I  want  Martin  to  show  me  a  vehicle  that  is  an  airplaine  .... 
Very  good.  I  want  Cynthia  to  show  me  a  vehicle  that  is  a  truck  .... 
Good.  I  Want  Juanita  to  show  me  a  vehicle  that  is  a  banana. 

Girl:  It's  not  a  vehicle. 

T:  No,  you're  right  and  I  couldn't  fool  you.  That's  very  good  ....  I 
want  Olga  to  show  me  the  vehicles  ....  Very  good.  She  pointed  to  all  of 
them  because  they  are  all  vehicles.  But  are  they  all  ships? 

C:No. 

T:  Are  they  all  airplanes? 

C:No. 

T:  Are  they  all  trucks? 

C:No. 

T;  What  are  they  all?  What  can  you  call  all  these  things? 

C:  Vehicles. 

T:  Vehicles,  That's  very  good. 

A  year  later,  when  the  children  are  5,  they  learn  that  vehicles  includes 
still  other  types  and  even  sub-types.  After  the  group  has  discussed  the 
function  of  a  passenger  train-it  carries  people  places-the  teacher  shows 
them  a  freight  train. 

Teacher:  What  kind  of  train  do  we  call  this  one? 

Child:  Where  arc  the  people? 

T:  There  are  no  people  on  this  train.  What  does  this  train  take? 

Children:  Food,  Gasoline  ...   . 

T:  Food,  gasoline.  What  else  could  this  train  take? 

Child:  A  refrigerator. 

T:  A  refrigerator.  You're  right.  This  is  a  freight  train.  Say  it  ....  Say  it, 
Gonzalo,  what  kind  of  train  is  this? 

Boy:  Freight  train. 

T:  Say  the  whole  sentence. 

Boy:  This  is  a  freight  train. 

T:  Very  good. 

51 


The  lessons  on  vehicles-and  on  the  other  units-provide  opportunities 
for  taking  up  language  differences  that  present  special  problems.  One  ol 
these  problems  has  to  do  with  differences  in  length;  largo  and  mas  largo  i*i 
Spanish  become,  when  translated  literally,  long  and  more  long  in  English 
Teacher;  This  train  is  short.  Say  it. 
Children:  This  train  is  short. 

T:  (pointing  to  picture  of  a  long  train):  Is  this  train  short? 
C:  No. 

T:  This  is  not  short.  So  what  could  we  say  about  this  tram? 
C:  It  is  long. 
T:  It  is  long.  Say  it. 
C:  It  is  long. 

T:  (pointing  to  picture  of  a  longer  train):  This  train  is 

C:  More  long.  Longer. 
T:  Longer.  Say  it,  This  train  is  longer. 
C:  This  train  is  longer. 

T:  (pointing  to  longest  train):  What  can  we  say  about  this  train? 
C:  More  longer. 

T:  No,  we're  going  to  say  this  train  is .... 
Child:  Longest. 

T:  Good.  Longest.  This  train  is  longest.  Say  it. 

Language  lessons  for  the  5-year-olds  also  include  a  review  of  prcposi 
tions  (the  children  learn,  for  example,  that  in  English  an  airplane  is  on  tin 
ground  but  in  the  sky)  and  a  sequence  of  questions  enabling  the  child  ti 
relate  present,  past,  a,nd  futme  tenses  (the  airplane  is  in  the  sky;  bcfor> 
that  it  was  on  the  ground;  when  the  airplane  on  the  ground  takes  off,  i 
will  be  in  the  sky). 


Expanded  Language  Program 

Complementing  the  language  lessons  in  all  three  years  is  what   th 

Center  and  the  Southwest  Educational  Development  Laboratory  call  "th 

expanded  language  program."  This  is  so  planned  as  to  arouse  the  chil 

dren's  interest  in  talking  about  the  topics  they  have  encountered  in  th 

language  lesson,  and  about  related  topics.  It  gives  them  practice  in  the  nci 

words-and  concepts  and  structures-and  at  the  same  time  requires  ther 

to  use  their  memories,  reasoning  powers,  and  imaginations.  An  EngHs' 

lesson  dealing  with  fruits,  for  example,  is  followed  by  a  discussion  of  thes 

and  other  foods,  The  teacher  gets  the  talk  started  by  asking  questions 

"ruits  do  you  peel  before  eating?  Does  an  orange  have  more  juic 

lanana?  What  would  happen  if  you  squeezed  a  banana?  And  UK 

i  language  activity  involves  the  cooking  of  rice  and  corn,  in  ortk 

wt-and  talk  about-how  cooking  affects  their  volume.  Food  a  Is 

0  help  teach  the  concepts-and  the  labels-of  hot  and  cold  £oa 
cornflakes)  and  big  and  little  (apples). 

rt  of  this  program,  books  pertaining  to  the  unit  being  studied  ai 

1  the  classroom  for  the  children's  use,  and  at  story  time  each  da 


the  teacher  reads  from  one  of  them.  If  the  lesson  unit  deals  with  houses, 
for  example,  the  teacher  reads  a  story  about  houses.  Then  the  children  are 
encouraged  to  talk  about  their  own  home,  or  a  friend's  home,  or  anything 
that  the  story  has  brought  to  mind.  Five-year-olds  may  spend  three  or 
four  periods  on  the  same  story.  The  first  day  they  may  just  look  at  the 
pictures  and  talk  about  what  may  be  happening.  The  second  day,  the 
teacher  reads  the  story  and  asks  the  children  to  compare  their  predictions 
with  what  actually  happened.  The  third  day  the  children  draw  a  picture— 
the  scene  or  event  they  liked  best-and  the  next  day  they  talk  about  it  to 
the  group. 

For  the  3-year-olds,  the  expanded  language  program  is  in  Spanish 
throughout  the  year;  for  the  4-year-olds,  much  of  it  is  in  English,  but  the 
story  period  remains  in  Spanish.  For  the  5-year-olds,  practically  eveiy- 
thing  is  in  English.  Daily  music  and  art  periods  also  serve  the  language 
program. 


Developing  Perceptual  Motor  Abilities 

Many  disadvantaged  children  reach  first  grade  deficient  not  only  in 
language  skills  but  also  in  perceptual  skills,  which  are  even  more  basic  to 
intellectual  growth.  The  Good  Samaritan  school  seeks  to  develop  these 
abilities  through  planned  daily  exercises. 

Training  in  attentiveness  and  auditory  skills  begins  during  the  first  week 
of  school,  when  the  3-year-olds  learn  a  rule  about  listening— that  when 
they  hear  the  bell,  which  is  used  to  signal  a  change  in  activities,  they  will 
stop  what  they  are  doing  and  listen.  Each  child  rings  the  bell  and  repeats 
the  rule.  Another  time  the  teacher  demonstrates  two  bells  having  different 
tonal  qualities;  then  the  children  close  their  eyes,  the  teacher  rings  one  of 
the  bells,  and  the  children  tell  her  which  one  it  is.  In  another  exercise  the 
teacher  plays  one  of  four  instruments-bell,  drum,  triangle,  or  cymbals- 
and  a  child  who  has  been  blindfolded  is  asked  to  walk  to  it.  Other  lessons 
deal  with  sounds  in  the  home -a  door  closing,  dishes  rattling,  a  window 
being  opened,  water  running.  Such  exercises  get  the  children  accustomed 
to  paying  attention  to  the  differences  between  sounds  and  to  locating  the 
source  of  a  sound.  Many  of  the  children,  from  crowded,  noisy  homes,  may 
have  learned  to  tune  out;  now  they  are  learning  to  tune  in.  Later  on,  more 
advanced  auditory  discrimination  lessons  will  sharpen  their  ability  to  de- 
tect and  recognize  differences  in  the  sounds  of  words, 

The  visual  training  program  includes  practice  in  discriminating  among 
objects  on  the  basis  of  size,  color,  shape,  or  function;  paying  attention  to 
the  boundaries  of  objects,  as  in  cutting  out  or  coloring  a  picture;  and 
noticing  and  reproducing  patterns,  as  with  blocks,  pegs,  and  beads.  Thus, 
the  program  strengthens  visual  skills  and  reasoning  ability  and  at  the  same 
time  helps  prepare  the  child  for  an  important  task  that  he  will  face  when 
learning  to  read-distinguishing  one  letter  from  another.  Under  visual 
training,  too,  come  many  exercises  to  strengthen  attentiveness  and  memo- 
ry. In  one,  for  example,  the  teacher  displays  three  pictures,  then  asks  the 

53 


children  to  close  their  eyes  as  she  turns  one  over.  Can  they  remember 
which  one  is  missing?  When  there  are  four  pictures,  can  they  remember^ 
Five?  Seven?  Another  exeicise  uses  letters  instead  of  pictures.  Another 
calls  on  the  children  to  reproduce  from  memory  patterns  they  have  been 
shown-simple  ones,  like  circles  and  triangles,  at  first;  later  on,  numbers 
and  letters. 

While  the  typical  disadvantaged  child  is  proficient  in  such  gross  motor 
activities  as  running,  jumping,  and  climbing,  he  lags  in  the  development  of 
the  fine  motor  skills  needed  for  classroom  success,  particularly  in  writing. 
The  Center's  daily  schedule,  therefore,  includes  such  activities  as  lacing 
shoes,  tieing  ribbons,  manipulating  small  building  blocks,  dropping  bill- 
tons  through  a  narrow  opening,  using  a  pair  of  scissois,  tracing,  coloring, 
and  pasting. 


Evaluating  the  Project 

By  the  time  the  children  have  completed  two  years  at  the  Center,  most 
of  them-whether  at  play  in  the  couityard  or  at  work  with  another  child 
on  some  project  in  the  classroom-are  using  both  Spanish  and  English. 
There  is  no  conscious  choice,  the  teachers  think;  the  children  use  the 
words  that  come  to  mind  first. 

Will  these  children,  after  the  third  pre-school  year,  be  able  to  compete 
in  first  grade  with  children  who  have  learned  English  at  home? 

The  staff  feels  that  they  will.  In  substantiation,  the  teacher  of  the 
5-year-olds  points  out  that  in  first  grade,  where  she  taught  for  four  years, 
teachers  are  supposed  to  begin  the  reading  readiness  program  immediately, 
a  very  difficult  matter  with  children  who  know  little  English.  The  5-ycar- 
olds  at  the  Center,  she  thinks,  are  ready  for  such  a  program  even  before 
they  have  finished  half  of  their  last  year.  They  have  been  trained  to  listen, 
which  is  one  of  the  goals  of  the  public  schools*  reading  readiness  program. 
They  have  learned  enough  English  to  be  able  to  follow  instructions;  for 
example,  they  know  when  the  teacher  is  talking  about  the  top  of  the  page 
and  when  she  is  talking  about  the  bottom.  They  have  learned  other  con- 
cepts and  the  English  words  for  them.  Give  some  first-graders  from  dis- 
advantaged homes  two  or  three  objects  and  ask  whether  they  are  the  same 
or  different,  and  the  children  will  just  look  at  the  teacher  or  say  "Maybe." 
But  the  children  at  the  Center,  she  says,  "can  tell  you  right  off."  The 
Center's  children  have  also  had  experience  in  a  common  reading-readiness 
exercise:  "Let's  look  at  this  picture  and  you  tell  me  what  you  see."  The 
Center's  children  have  done  this  many  times;  they  know  the  labels  i» 
English-  "boy,"  "girl,"  "father,"  "mother,"  "house,"  "car"-for  what 
the  pictures  show,  and  they  have  had  practice  in  expressing  themselves. 

Approximately  30  of  the  Center's  graduates  are  now  in  first  or  second 
grade,  in  half  a  dozen  different  schools.  Two  teachers  have  spoken  highly 
of  the  few  they  have  encountered,  and  at  least  a  dozen  mothers  have 
dropped  by  the  Center  to  report  that  the  Good  Samaritan  graduates  are 
doing  considerably  better  in  school  than  their  older  brothers  and  sisters. 

54 


For  lack  of  funds,  the  Center  notes,  a  systematic  follow-up  of  these 
children  has  not  yet  been  undertaken.  However,  under  a  ptogram  financed 
by  the  Southwest  Educational  Development  Laboratory,  an  effort  has 
been  made  to  evaluate  the  San  Antonio  project  on  the  basis  of  (a)  changes 
in  the  children  attending  its  classes  during  the  1968-69  school  year,  and 
(b)  a  comparison  of  these  changes  with  those  experienced  by  children  in 
two  other  groups.  One  of  these  groups  came  from  three  day-care  centers 
in  San  Antonio  funded  as  Head  Start  programs  undei  the  Office  of  Eco- 
nomic Opportunity  and  concerned  with  children  from  Mexican-American 
families  of  low-economic  status.  These  centers  offered  some  of  the  ele- 
ments of  the  Good  Samaritan  program  but  were  concerned  in  the  main 
with  providing  all-day  care  and  supervised  play.  The  second  comparison 
group  comprised  16  3-year-olds  who  were  eligible  for  the  Good  Samari- 
tan's regular  program  but  could  not  be  admitted.  The  parents  of  these 
children  were  encouraged  to  participate  in  a  parent-involvement  program; 
through  it,  the  Center  hoped  to  learn  whether  or  not  the  children  could  be 
affected  indirectly  by  increasing  the  parents'  interest  in  child  develop- 
ment. As  it  turned  out,  the  fathers  and  mothers  involved  in  this  program 
attended  the  scheduled  semi-monthly  meetings  quite  irregularly,  and  the 
discussions— though  they  included  such  topics  as  hygiene,  mental  health, 
and  story-telling  techniques-were  not  so  specifically  concerned  with  child 
development  as  had  been  planned.  Essentially,  the  children  in  this  group 
may  be  considered  controls— that  is,  as  having  experienced  no  significant 
intervention. 

To  try  to  determine  the  intellectual  development  of  the  children  in  the 
three  groups  during  the  nine  months  between  September  1968  and  May 
1969,  the  staff  used: 

1 .  The  Leiter  International  Performance  Scale,  a  non-language  test  rely- 
ing heavily  on  visual  discrimination.  Some  items  call  for  matching  one 
object  with  another;  others,  for  grouping  objects  that  belong  together.  The 
examiner  demonstrates  what  is  to  be  done—for  example,  he  takes  a  red 
block  and  puts  it  with  a  red  square,  Then  he  gives  the  child  other  items  in 
the  same  category  to  do  by  himself. 

2.  The  Peabody  Picture  Vocabulary  Test,  which  provides  an  estimate 
of  the  child's  verbal  intelligence  although  the  child  himself  is  not  required 
to  talk.  The  examiner  names  an  object  (such  as  "dog")  and  then  asks  the 
child  to  point  to  it  in  one  of  several  pictures  presented  to  him.  In  addition 
to  the  standard  English  version  of  this  test,  the  Center  developed  and  used 
a  Spanish  version. 

The  results  are  shown  in  Table  1 . 

In  the  first  examination,  as  expected,  since  all  the  children  came  from 
educationally  deprived,  non-English-speaking  homes,  all  three  groups 
scored  well  below  the  national  average  in  a  test -the  Peabody-requiring 
the  use  of  English  in  its  administration.  Each  group  fell  below  the  average 
by  at  least  40  points.  Even  in  the  Spanish  version  of  this  test,  the  groups 
scored  from  12  to  25  points  below  standard.  On  the  instrument  not 
requiring  language  in  its  administration-the  Leiter— all  three  groups  fell 
within  the  normal  range.  This  result,  too,  had  been  expected. 

55 


Table  I 

AVERAGE  SCORES  BEFORE  AND  AFTER  NINE  MONTH  TRIAL   PERIOD 
Test  Good  Samaritan  Day  Care  "Controls"* 

Sept.  May  Sept.  May  Sept.  Moy 

Uiter  107  121"  99  101  97  96 


Peabody- 
English 

59                68 

60 

63 

58 

58 

Peabody- 
Spanish 

88              102*" 

76 

77 

75 

80 

"Children  in  Parent-Involvement  program.  See  text, 

"Significant  at  less  than  the  01  level,  meaning  that  the  results  would  have  occurred  by  chnncolt 
than  1  time  in  TOO. 

Nine  months  later,  only  the  children  who  had  attended  the  Good  Samar 

itan  Program  showed  marked  gains.  On  the  English  version  of  the  Peabodj 

Test,  this  gain  was  not-by  a  slight  margin -statistically  significant;  on  the 

Spanish  version  and  on  the  non-verbal  test,  it  was.  The  scores  on  these 

tests  were  also  significantly  greater  than  those  made  by  the  other  groups. 

In  sum,  the  Center's  program  for  3-year-olds  significantly  increased 

their  intellectual  performance  as  compared  with  what  it  had  been  and  as 

compared  with  the  performance  of  children  in  a  traditional  nursery  school 

program  and  with  that  of  children  not  involved  in  a  program. 

The  staff  emphasizes  that  these  findings  are  for  a  single  year  only  and 
for  small  samples.  It  points  out,  too,  that  the  evaluation  tests  used  so  far 
are  not  ideal  by  any  means.  Though  the  Leiter  eludes  the  language  barrier, 
the  test  is  time-consuming  and  those  who  administer  it  may  unwittingly 
provide  clues  to  answers  unless  specially  trained.  The  experience  at  San 
Antonio  suggests  that  the  vocabulary  of  the  Peabody  Picture  Vocabulary 
Test  is  too  advanced  for  disadvantaged  pre-school  children  and  contains 
too  few  items  at  each  age  level  for  accurate  measurement  of  differences 
between  groups, 

Improved  evaluation  measures.  In  conjunction  with  child  development 
authorities  in  Texas  universities  and  the  public  school  system*  the  South- 
west Educational  Development  Laboratory  is  developing  and  standard- 
izing several  tests  intended  to  measure  a  pre-school  child's  proficiency  in 
language  more  adequately  than  tests  now  availnhlfl.  Onp.  snnh  effort  is 


Working  with  Parents 

The  Center  keeps  in  touch  with  parents  through  monthly  meetings  to 
which  all  fathers  and  mothers  are  invited  and  through  conferences  with 
individuals  when  desnable.  The  meetings  are  quite  informal,  almost  like 
family  gatherings;  mothers  even  bring  their  new  babies  to  be  admired.  At 
one  recent  session,  the  talk  got  around  to  how  children  ought  to  behave 
when  they  got  to  public  school.  One  mother  expressed  the  opinion  that  the 
teacher  was  always  right.  "Keep  quiet,"  she  said  she  told  her  fiist-grader; 
"then  the  teacher  is  going  to  like  you,  and  you'll  pass  into  second  grade." 
The  other  parents  seemed  to  agree.  Mrs.  Blankenship  suggested,  though, 
that  it  was  healthy  for  a  child  to  ask  questions,  that  he  had  a  right  to  ask 
them,  and  that,  in  fact,  a  teacher  wouid  like  him  to  ask  them. 

When  a  problem  arises  that  the  Center  alone  cannot  meet,  the  people 
who  know  the  child  and  his  family— generally  at  least  one  teacher,  hei 
assistant,  and  Mrs.  Blankenship-talk  it  over  and  decide  how  to  advise  the 
parents.  Usually  the  mother  will  be  asked  to  drop  in;  sometimes  a  staff 
member  will  visit  the  home.  The  mother  of  an  extremely  timid  girl  was 
advised  to  scold  her  less,  praise  her  more,  and  give  her  some  opportunity 
to  talk.  The  parents  of  a  boy  who  had  no  motivation  because  at  home  his 
older  brothers  and  sisters  did  everything  for  him,  were  advised  to  let  him 
have  more  responsibility  and  to  praise  him  whenever  he  undertook  a  new 
task. 

Parent  Education  Program.  This  program,  a  revision  of  the  parental 
involvement  project  noted  earlier,  is  for  16  mothers  of  3-year-olds  who  are 
not  included  in  the  regular  pre-school  course.  At  the  start,  each  mother 
was  video-taped  as  she  taught  her  child  simple  tasks,  such  as  sorting 
blocks,  and  as  she  read  to  him,  The  staff  wanted  to  learn  how  well  the 
mothers  explained  a  task  to  their  children,  how  they  organized  informa- 
tion, to  what  extent  they  used  praise,  and  how  much  affection  there  was 
between  mother  and  child.  Now  the  Center  is  trying  to  train  the  mothers 
to  work  with  their  children  more  effectively.  This  involves  showing  them 
how  they  can  present  educational  activities,  such  as  comparing  fruits  of 
different  color  and  size  and  labeling  and  counting  common  household 
objects.  In  many  cases  it  also  involves  changing  certain  behavior  patterns, 
the  most  common  of  which  is  to  ignore  good  behavior  and  punish  bad 
behavior,  so  that  the  only  way  a  child  gets  attention  from  his  mother  is  to 
misbehave, 

Mrs.  Blankenship,  working  with  mothers  at  the  Center,  demonstrates  a 
different  activity  each  week.  Once  a  week  another  staff  member  visits  the 
homes  and  observes  how  the  mother  works  with  her  child  and  whether  or 
not  the  child  has  learned  the  activity  for  the  week.  The  Center  plans  to 
continue  this  program  for  three  years  so  that  it  can  compaie  the  effects  of 
working  directly  with  children  in  the  classroom  and  working  indirectly 
with  them  through  their  mothers.  Evidence  that  the  children  of  the 
mothers  in  the  Parent  Education  Program  are  substantially  benefitted 
would  be  welcome,  would  point  toward  a  way  of  reaching  educationally 

57 


The  program  is  double-barreled  in  that  it  helps  a  group  of  deprived,  social- 
ly disadvantaged,  poverty-inhibited  adolescents  who  then,  each  in  turn, 
provide  a  significant  mental  health  service  to  the  community.  Help  is  given 
at  less  cost  and  more  effectively  than  it  could  be  given  by  a  progiam 
limited  to  traditional  use  of  professionals 

The  area  has  an  acute  lack  of  clinical  facilities  for  children  and  adoles- 
cents. However,  it  is  obvious  that  the  resources  of  such  facilities,  primarily 
oriented  as  they  are  to  the  middle  class,  would  be  inadequate  to  the  job 
anyway.  The  usual  agency  is  not  oriented  to  the  real  problems  of  the 
poor,  and  the  poor  genei  ally  do  not  seek  its  services.  Treatment  personnel, 
drawn  from  the  middle  class,  are  not  in  time  with  the  clients  and  find  it 
difficult  to  work  with  them  Theie  is  a  special  need  for  integration  of  any 
treatment  program  into  the  local  institutions,  i.e.,  the  schools  and  courts, 
and  this  generally  is  not  done. 

Another  problem  is  that  in  this  type  of  neighborhood  the  impact  of 
treatment  may  be  compromised  by  the  stigma  of  being  a  patient.  At  the 
same  time,  the  undeistanding  and  knowledge  of  the  mental  health  practi- 
tioner is  valuable,  and  it  is  primarily  a  new  approach  that  is  needed.  One 
theory  (held  also  by  the  staff  at  Baker's  Dozen)  is  that  the  best  kinds  of 
prevention  and  therapy  aie  those  which  can  be  done  in  the  normal  social 
context  of  the  youth-that  is,  within  the  schools,  the  work,  or  the  natural 
groups  of  the  community. 

In  addition,  the  services  must  be  realistically  accessible.  For  instance,  at 
Baker's  Dozen  a  variety  of  services  are  offered  because  there  are  so  few 
facilities  in  the  neighborhood.  The  services  are  free,  as  the  clients  have  no 
money.  Baker's  Dozen  has  no  authority  in  the  sense  that  the  police  or 
schools  have  authority,  and  so  it  must  attract  and  hold  boys  and  girls  by 
the  opportunity  for  change,  for  friendships,  for  the  activities  that  it  offers. 
It  must  be  open  the  hours  that  it  is  needed,  not  just  9-5  on  weekdays. 
Therefore  its  schedule  is  extended  to  evenings  and  weekends. 

Aides,  the  term  used  for  the  young  indigenous  workers,  were  recruited 
and  selected  through  a  process  of  "screening  in"  rather  than  "screening 
out/1  Signs,  encouraging  people  to  apply,  were  put  up  in  a  wide  variety  of 
places  including  bars  and  laundromats.  Radio  and  TV  news  announce- 
ments were  made.  Applicants  were  considered  with  characteristics  that 
would  ordinarily  bar  them  from  employment.  Only  a  fifth-grade  educa- 
tion was  required  and  this  only  so  that  reading  would  be  possible.  No 
previous  work  experience  was  necessary.  Most  jobs  call  for  a  clean  police 
record,  but  this  was  not  required  here.  It  was  essential,  however,  that  no 
court  action  be  pending  which  would  interrupt  the  training.  As  to 
personal  characteristics,  the  only  requirements  were  that  applicants  be 
free  of  serious  physical  or  mental  problems  and  communicable  diseases. 
Psychological  testing  was  used  to  identify  gaps  and  problems  and  was 
referred  to  again  later  when  an  evaluation  of  the  training  was  made. 

Applicants  were  encouraged  from  low  socioeconomic  groups  and  from 
families  with  low  incomes.  Five  men  and  four  women  were  selected,  all  of 
whom  had  lived  within  five  blocks  of  the  Center.  They  ranged  in  age  from 
17  to  21.  Several  had  dropped  out  of  school  around  the  seventh  grade; 

60 


one  had  completed  high  school.  The  reading  level  ranged  from  a  minimum 
of  fifth  grade  to  a  maximum  level  of  llth  grade.  One  young  man  was 
diopped  because  of  legal  problems. 

The  eight  trainees  were  subdivided  into  high-  and  low-risk  groups,  four 
in  each.  High-risk  youths  were  described  as  deprived  youths  who  had  had 
a  series  of  police  and  criminal  involvements,  some  emotional  or  delin- 
quency problems,  and  those  who  may  have  spent  time  in  an  institution  for 
an  offense.  They  read  at  a  minimal  fifth-grade  level,  dropped  out  of  school 
early,  worked  only  at  odd  jobs,  and  never  worked  longer  than  3  months  at 
any  given  job.  The  low-risk  youths  were  defined  as  deprived  youths  who 
had  no  police  record,  who  continued  in  school  until  family  and  poverty 
circumstances  forced  them  out.  They  worked  at  menial  jobs  but  for  longer 
periods  than  did  the  high-risk  group.  The  average  number  of  siblings  for 
the  aides  as  a  group  was  five.  All  the  aides  chosen  had  multiple  social 
problems  and  were  so  accustomed  to  rejection,  failure,  and  defeat  that 
they  had  to  be  convinced  that  all  this  was  true.  They  suspected  that 
"there  must  be  a  catch  to  it  somewhere." 

Recruits  were  numerous,  and  the  rate  of  completion  throughout  this 
project  and  otheis  run  by  the  Institute  for  Youth  Studies  was  very  high. 
The  staff  reports  that  over  1 50  multiproblem  youths  have  been  trained  in 
Institute  for  Youth  Studies  projects  and  that  the  dropout  rate  has  been 
less  than  1  percent.3  One  aide  selected  for  the  Baker's  Dozen  project  was 
dropped  as  he  had  to  serve  a  prison  term,  and  at  this  writing  he  had  still 
not  returned  to  the  project.  This  points  up  how  strong  the  holding  power 
of  the  project  has  been  as  many  of  these  youths  would  have  undoubtedly 
been  in  police  trouble  and  perhaps  given  stiffer  sentences  due  to  their 
histories  of  past  offenses.  In  this  and  other  ways  they  might  have  been 
prevented  from  continuing  in  the  project  had  they  not  exercised  extreme 
caution  in  order  to  be  able  to  participate. 

Applicants  were  seen  in  groups  during  the  selection  process.  Every 
effort  was  made  not  to  impose  unnecessary  barriers  that  would  cause 
people  to  drop  out.  Even  so,  one  aide  said  at  a  later  date  that  he  had 
arrived  at  the  Center  and  found  instructions  to  go  to  Freedman's  Hospital 
for  a  physical,  As  he  didn't  have  busfare,  he  almost  quit  until  a  staff 
member  organized  rides  for  the  women  and  encouraged  the  men  to  walk 
there  together,  thus  solving  the  problem. 

The  initial  training  period  lasted  3  months.  When  it  was  completed,  a 
graduation  was  held  that  was  the  fiist  such  ceremony  most  of  them  had 
ever  attended  and  marked  for  many  the  longest  period  of  employment  to 
date.  The  aides  were  given  a  stipend  of  $20  a  week  during  the  training 
period;  this  jumped  to  $75  a  week  at  the  end  of  that  time.  Presently  they 
are  earning  $80  a  week,  and  increases  are  written  into  the  budget.  They 
are  considered  GS-2  level  ($4,108),  and  provision  has  been  made  that  they 
will  move  up  to  Government  Service  levels  3  and  4.  The  District  of 
Columbia  Health  Department  has  written  job  descriptions  and  positions 


2These  aides  are  working  in  schools,  settlement  houses,  and  children's  institution 
recreational  centers  throughout  the  city. 

61 


into  their  budget,  and  this  new  type  of  position  will  be  continued  after 
the  initial  demonstration  phase  has  ended.  An  exciting  part  of  the  pro- 
gram has  been  the  development  of  new  caieers  and  the  additional  resource 
for  manpower  in  mental  health. 

Training  Program  for  the  Aides 

Major  training  goals  were: 

1 .  Development  in  these  youths  of  the  necessary  motivation,  identity, 
values,  and  capabilities  for  maximally  utilizing  the  offered  training. 

2.  Leaining  the  basic  personal,  social,  and  interpersonal  skills,  attitudes, 
and  knowledge  which  would  help  them  successfully  cope  with  and 
solve  group,  client,  and  personal  problems. 

3 .  Learning  specialized  skills  for  their  roles  in  mental  health. 

4.  Developing  flexibility  of  attitude,  role,  and  viewpoint. 

Training  was  designed  in  three  parts  which  ran  concurrently -core  group, 
specialty  workshop,  and  on-the-job  training.  These  were  continued  after 
the  initial  3-month  training  period  and  comprise  the  vehicle  for  inservice 
training  which  continues  throughout  the  program. 


Core  Group 

This  is  the  basic  group  which  meets  several  hours  a  week  and  is  led  by 
the  same  professional  staff  person.  It  provides  a  place  where  a  variety  of 
issues  can  be  raised  and  day-to-day  problems  of  work  can  be  discussed. 
The  group  itself  provides  a  laboratory  for  group  interaction  and  manage- 
ment techniques.  Work  throughout  the  training  program  is  done  primarily 
with  the  entire  group.  It  has  been  found  that  this  makes  possible  greater 
participation  on  the  part  of  the  aides  as  they  have  the  support  of  their 
peers. 

The  level  of  anxiety  among  the  aides  was  high  due  to  the  new  demands 
made  upon  them,  and  the  group  was  found  to  be  effective  in  dealing  with 
this  in  a  way  that  allowed  for  maximum  change.  It  gave  each  aide  a  group 
to  move  with  in  his  transition  from  an  unemployed  school  dropout  to  a 
semi-professional.  This  is  very  important  because  these  young  people, 
drawn  away  from  familiar  patterns  and  attitudes,  often  feel  very  isolated. 
They  have  helped  each  other  to  correct  the  distorted  views  they  have  held 
as  a  result  of  their  background.  They  have  learned  new  ways  of  solving 
problems,  often  by  applying  the  new  techniques  to  problems  that  come 
up  in  the  core  group.  The  leader  is  a  visible  example  of  how  to  lead  a 
group;  what  he  does  is  more  likely  to  be  influential  than  anything  said  in  a 
lecture. 

The  trainees  and  the  staff  have  had  some  difficulty  in  establishing  the 
role  of  the  aides.  For  instance,  such  questions  arise  as  to  how  much  the 
aides  should  have  to  say  in  policy  decisions.  How  much  responsibility 
should  they  have?  The  core  group  was  used  to  define  an  appropriate  job 

62 


identification.  Initially,  the  aides  wondered,  "Should  I  be  like  the  Man  or 
myself,"  Although  they  were  encouraged  to  absorb  attitudes  and  tech- 
niques, they  were  discouraged  from  becoming  carbon  copies  of  the  staff  as 
it  was  hoped  that  a  new  type  of  professional,  comfortable  with  lower  class 
behavioi  and  at  the  same  time  able  to  meet  middle-class  demands,  would 
emerge.  Since  it  was  highly  desirable  that  they  remain  in  touch  with  and 
empathic  to  the  lower-class  group  from  which  they  came,  this  was  en- 
couraged. Their  observations  about  the  shortcomings  of  professional  tech- 
niques were  encouraged.  The  staff  had  to  learn  to  stand  the  criticism  and 
separate  the  useful  information  from  the  aggressive  attack. 

The  aides  were  also  encouraged  to  examine  their  own  backgrounds. 
With  the  help  of  their  leader  they  developed  a  very  perceptive  paper, 
"What  It  Means  To  Be  Really  Poor."  This  was  an  outgrowth  of  their 
examination  of  the  process  of  adolescence  and  the  difficulties  of  living  in 
a  slum.  They  began  to  acquire  some  perspective  and  to  sort  out  what 
could  be  done.  In  their  paper  they  comment  that  when  you  are  poor  you 
know  only  your  own  troubles  and  your  own  neighborhood.3  If  you  are 
failing  in  school  you  do  not  have  the  time  to  ponder  what  it  is  about 
American  education  that  makes  it  difficult  for  the  lower  income  student 
to  achieve  scholastically.  Nor  do  you  have  the  energy  to  do  something 
about  the  situation. 

In  the  core  gioup,  methods  of  social  action  are  discussed,  and  the  aides 
have  been  encouraged  to  participate  in  community  groups.  They  have 
attended  schoolboard  meetings  and  Senate  hearings.  They  have  been  en- 
couraged by  the  staff  to  speak  up  regardless  of  whether  or  not  other 
groups  in  the  community  find  their  opinions  embarrassing.  A  prominent 
issue  for  these  trainees  have  been  that  of  discrimination.  All  are  expected 
to  have  opinions  and  to  act  accordingly.  They  are  expected  to  act 
responsibly  and  knowledgeably.  Early  in  the  IYS  training  programs  there 
was  discussion  as  to  whether  trainees  should  attend  community  meetings 
as  observers.  This  was  found  to  be  confusing  and  tended  to  weaken  the 
piogram  and  was  abandoned.  A  major  problem  of  these  young  people  has 
been  their  attitude  of  defeated,  resigned  acceptance  of  an  unsatisfactory 
status  quo,  and  group  action  protest  methods  have  been  helpful  in  showing 
them  ways  to  achieve  improvement. 

Tiie  issues  discussed  are  varied.  At  one  core  meeting  questions  were 
raised  about  vacation  hours,  a  secretary's  attitude,  and  what  to  do  about  a 
letter  received  from  an  invalid  old  lady  who  wanted  a  Christmas  basket. 
(Although  this  is  not  a  routine  agency  service,  the  group  had  already 
called  her  and  taken  up  a  collection.)  They  also  discussed  whether  or  not 
an  aide,  despite  provocation,  should  ever  hit  one  of  the  children  in  his 
group.  Other  issues,  such  as  the  use  of  the  credit  union,  are  raised  by  the 
leader.  The  staff  reports  that  a  great  deal  of  information  taken  for  granted 
by  middle-class  workers  has  to  be  discussed  with  the  aides  in  order  to  be 
sure  that  they  clearly  understand  both  their  rights  and  their  obligations. 


3  "What  It  Means  To  Be  Really  Poor."  Baker's  Dozen  aides  and  Lonnie  Mitchell, 
Ph.D,  Baker's  Dozen  Youth  Center,  Washington,  D.C.,  unpublished  paper. 

63 


The  job  of  the  leader  of  the  coie  group  is  a  difficult  one.  The  young 
people  aie  angry,  suspicious,  distrustful,  vulnerable,  hopeful,  sensitive, 
brutally  realistic,  and  terribly  unrealistic  all  at  the  same  time.  There  is  a 
gap  that  must  be  spanned,  Neither  side  wants  to  be  compromised,  T!\c 
leader  must  be  comfortable  with  authority  and  know  when  to  turn 
responsibility  over  to  the  group,  He  must  act  as  a  liaison  with  the  staff.  He 
must  interpret  the  staff  to  aides  and  vice  versa.  He  must  be  a  good  and 
knowledgeable  teacher.  The  group  will  scrutinize  him  and  be  critical  as  he 
represents  the  people  trying  to  remake  them.  He  is  the  personification  of 
the  professional.  All  staff  working  with  the  aides  must  be  able  to  handle 
passivity  without  becoming  dictatorial.  If  the  leader  or  other  staff  mem- 
bers become  anxious  about  group  inactivity  or  lack  of  response,  tlic 
group's  confidence  in  their  own  ability  will  be  undermined.  A  good  ex* 
ample  of  this  occurred  in  an  interview  with  the  group.  After  an  initial 
explanation  of  the  broad  questions  to  be  discussed,  the  group  was  utterly 
unresponsive.  The  leader  then  restated  the  issues.  The  group  remained 
unresponsive.  Then  upon  questioning  they  said  that  they  found  the 
second  explanation  insulting  because  it  made  them  look  stupid.  Upon 
hearing  that  a  response  is  looked  for  as  evidence  of  understanding,  they 
said  that  that  was  "middle  class"  and  asked  who  wants  to  "react  and 
commit  themselves."  The  staff  reported  that  complaints  about  the  pro- 
gram and  lack  of  response  were  common  manifestations  of  anxiety  and 
feelings  of  noninvolvement  in  the  program. 

The  staff  reports  also  that  they  have  found  a  rather  strict  approach  as 
opposed  to  a  permissive  one  most  effective.  Rules  are  strict,  but  anything 
is  open  to  discussion.  Aides  are  docked  for  being  late  even  if  it  is  a  small 
amount.  Hours  are  checked.  A  great  deal  is  asked  of  the  aides  in  terms  of 
meeting  professional  standards.  The  aides  by  and  large  have  been  able  to 
do  this.  On  the  other  hand,  the  project  has  not  discharged  aides  for  some 
issues,  angry  episodes,  minor  police  violations,  etc.,  that  would  be  suf- 
ficient grounds  for  dismissal  in  some  agencies.  The  fact  that  the  finances 
are  administered  through  a  university  has  made  its  employment  standards 
binding  on  the  aides  and  has  imposed  stricter  rules  than  the  staff  would 
have  wished.  These,  however,  have  been  subject  to  appeal. 

The  aides  are  interested  in  the  process  of  becoming  a  professional.  Most 
of  them  have  now  grasped  the  concept  of  the  steps  leading  to  a  profession 
and  have  either  gotten  tutoring  or  resumed  special  classes  in  school.  They 
now  see  that  it  is  not  magic  which  makes  a  person  a  doctor,  a  psy- 
chologist, or  a  social  worker,  but  a  process  pursued  step  by  step.  The 
program  in  general  and  the  staff  personally  have  given  support  to  any 
interest  in  further  education  on  the  part  of  the  aides.  One  boy  audits 
classes  in  psychology  at  Howard.  They  are  also  encouraged  to  continually 
relate  the  more  academic  work  in  their  training  to  their  own  lives.  The 
aides  have  commented  on  this  issue  in  their  paper:4  "The  learning  en- 
vironment of  the  slum  child  is  dismal.  He  is  often  emotionally  disturbed. 
It  is  a  mistake  to  urge  such  a  child  to  get  an  education  because  it  will  help 


"What  It  Means  To  Be  Really  Poor." 
64 


him  to  get  a  good  job  and  allow  him  to  leave  the  slums.  The  most 
promising  motivation  for  a  child  in  the  culture  of  the  poor  is  the  acquisi- 
tion of  knowledge  as  an  end  in  itself  and  for  its  own  sake."  The  irony  is 
that,  having  found  a  good  job,  the  aides  have  found  their  own  educations 
too  limited  to  allow  them  sure  access  to  better  jobs.  One  young  woman, 
married  and  with  two  children,  would  like  to  be  a  social  worker.  She  is 
young  but  would  have  to  complete  high  school  and  go  on  to  college,  and 
remedial  work  takes  more  time  than  original  schooling  did  as  it  must  be 
done  around  the  demands  of  other  responsibilities.  This,  coupled  with  the 
lack  of  money,  poses  almost  insurmountable  problems  despite  her  intel- 
ligence and  abilities. 


Specialty  Workshops 

The  aides  were  given  didactic  work  on  such  subjects  as  interviewing, 
history  taking,  record  keeping,  group  observations,  psychological  testing, 
etc.  The  classes  resemble  college  survey  courses  in  style,  They  sought  to 
provide  an  overview.  Needless  to  say,  the  aides  needed  a  great  many 
background  issues  filled  in.  The  staff  opeiated  by  giving  talks  and  lectures 
and  utilizing  extensive  questioning  and  give-and-take  to  be  certain  that  the 
information  was  being  understood  and  absorbed.  The  aides  often  brought 
up  questions  about  such  things  as  the  effects  of  LSD,  alcoholism,  etc.  The 
staff  talked  about  issues  reported  in  the  newspaper  and  tried  to  keep 
stimulating  the  group  but  tried  at  the  same  time  not  to  ovei  whelm  the 
group.  The  investigators  report  that  it  was  necessary  to  keep  a  current 
tone  to  the  work  because  the  group  as  a  whole  had  such  a  negative  feeling 
about  schoolwork  that  initially  they  found  it  almost  intolerable,  The 
problem  of  even  sitting  stilf  was  difficult  for  some  to  manage.  They  had 
felt  so  out  of  touch  with  previous  teachers,  so  attacked  and  so  unsuccess- 
ful, that  it  posed  a  difficult  teaching  task  to  the  staff  to  create  an 
atmosphere  which  could  foster  their  learning.  Efforts  were  made  also  to 
pace  the  work  so  that  the  aides  would  learn  answers  to  problems  that 
arose  in  their  groups  so  the  practical  significance  of  the  theory  would  be 
visible. 


On-the-Job  Training 

Many  visits  to  community  agencies  were  also  part  of  the  training.  The 
group  visited  Congress,  the  juvenile  court,  Junior  Village  (a  children's 
residential  home),  St.  Elizabeths  Hospital  (a  psychiatric  facility),  and 
other  social  institutions.  They  saw  many  films  on  mental  health  and  child 
development.  They  interviewed  members  of  the  neighborhood  for  ideas 
about  community  needs,  and  they  learned  the  rudiments  of  research 
methods. 

Throughout  the  training  it  was  intended  that  the  aides  would  learn  to 
become  more  sensitive  to  interpersonal  feelings.  They  report  an  awareness 

65 


of  a  considerable  change  in  how  much  they  perceive  of  one  another's 
feelings.  They  cite  this  as  a  problem  in  the  training  as  sometimes  they  feel 
awkward  and  ill-equipped  to  deal  with  this.  This  problem  is  familiar  to 
anyone  who  has  experienced  similar  training.  Although  any  staff  member 
would  talk  to  any  aide  who  sought  his  help,  there  has  also  been  a  group 
psychotherapy  program  for  the  aides.  This  makes  it  possible  for  issues 
inappropriate  to  other  meetings  to  be  referred  back  to  that  group.  The 
same  psychiatrist  has  met  with  the  group  since  their  entrance  into  the 
program.  He  feels,  as  does  the  rest  of  the  staff,  that  the  aides  have  over- 
come some,  but  not  all,  of  their  initial  difficulty  in  talking  about  their 
feelings-an  idea  quite  alien  to  their  way  of  life.  They  were,  as  a  group* 
much  more  comfortable  with  activity  and  movement  than  with  verbal 
modes  of  expression. 

There  is  a  fear  that  you  can  be  manipulated  when  people  know  your 
feelings.  The  aides  talk  about  "gritting,"  which  means  maintaining  silence 
as  a  way  of  controlling  a  situation  and  still  remaining  technically  non- 
obstructive.  The  staff  has  learned  to  call  the  aides  on  such  tactics  and  lias 
earned  the  respect  of  the  aides  to  the  extent  that  they  have  done  this.  The 
psychiatrist  who  meets  with  the  group  makes  no  administrative  decisions 
about  them  because  it  was  hoped  that  this  would  further  a  therapeutic 
atmosphere  and  provide  one  place  where  the  aides  could  speak  without 
fear  of  losing  their  jobs.  At  times  of  acute  personal  stress  the  aides  have 
been  referred  to  other  psychiatrists  in  the  community,  The  group  meets  at 
Baker's  Dozen,  and  it  is  considered  part  of  their  program  and  is  com 
pulsory. 

Basically  the  aides  were  trained  to  carry  out  specific  duties  and  to  fill 
certain  roles.  These  included; 

1 .  Be  group  leader,  helper,  and  planner  for  1 0  children  in  each  of  two 
groups. 

2.  Participate  with  the  psychologist,  psychiatrist,  or  social  worker  in 
developing  structured  therapeutic  programs  foi  their  groups. 

3.  Observe  and  record  individual  and  group  behavior. 

4.  Conduct  interviews  with  group  members  and  provide  information  to 
the  professional  staff  for  feedback  and  quality-control  purposes. 

5.  Escort  groups  on  trips  and  tours, 

6-  Participate  in  individual  and  group  supervision. 

7.  Attend  staff  conferences. 

8.  Write  progress  reports  and  keep  records  of  daily  observations  on  the 
children  with  whom  they  work. 

The  Groups  Led  by  the  Aides 

Each  aide  is  responsible  for  two  activity  groups.  Activities  are  struc- 
tured to  provide  ego-strengthening  and  therapeutic  benefits  and  include 
recreation  and  cultural,  social,  and  community  activities.  A  major  purpose 
is  to  raise  the  behavioral  standards  of  the  children  involved.  A  strong 
emphasis  is  placed  on  the  concept  of  the  aide  as  a  good  figure  for  the 


group  to  identify  with  and  on  the  use  of  the  aide's  management  to  estab- 
lish beneficial  controls  over  the  childien.  The  aides  seek  to  reduce 
symptoms,  lessen  police  contacts,  and  improve  the  social  functioning  of 
the  children.  The  children,  who  are  from  the  area  around  the  Center,  had 
school  problems,  difficulties  with  the  police,  defiant  attitudes  toward 
authority,  and  many  symptoms  of  social  and  personal  disorganization. 
Referrals  were  accepted  from  the  juvenile  courts,  public  schools,  the 
Urban  League,  and  other  agencies.  The  age  range  was  12—16.  Both  boys 
and  girls  were  accepted.  Youths  excluded  were  those  who  were  being 
committed  to  an  institution  as  a  result  of  court  action,  those  in  need  of 
immediate  hospitalization,  and  mental  defectives  whose  difficulties  posed 
certain  management  problems  in  an  outpatient  setting.  In  addition,  the 
aides  ran  dances  and  open  houses  to  bring  children  into  the  Center  and 
interest  them  in  the  program.  This  further  tended  to  bring  in  natural 
groupings  of  children.  Although  these  children  were  not  unusual  in  the 
community  and  not  technically  referred,  they  had  many  of  the  same 
characteristics  as  those  who  were.  This  points  up  the  degree  of  social 
disorganization  in  the  community  when  so  many  multiproblem  children 
can  be  found  by  simply  dipping  into  the  community.  Speaking  of  one 
such  child  an  aide  said,  "When  I  say  he  gave  the  secretary  a  hard  time,  I 
mean  he  pulled  a  gun  on  her." 

The  emphasis  is  on  work  with  the  teenager  rather  than  the  parent.  The 
aides  know  from  their  own  experiences  that  work  with  the  parents  would 
be  less  fruitful  than  work  with  the  children.  They  feel  a  positive  non- 
parent-connected  relationship  is  helpful  to  these  adolescents.  They  are 
only  too  well  aware  of  the  hostility  and  rejection  in  the  home  situations 
of  many  of  their  group  members.  They  point  out  that  these  young  people 
would  be  on  the  streets  if  they  were  not  at  the  Center.  The  importance  of 
"someplace  where  nobody  yells  at  you"  is  underestimated  in  the  aides' 
opinion.  Neither  the  aides  nor  the  group  members  see  this  as  a  patient- 
therapist  relationship.  They  see  it  as  working  together  toward  getting 
along,  and  planning  activities.  The  aides  are  rather  permissive  but  have 
certain  taboos,  such  as  not  allowing  the  boys  to  play  the  "dozens."5  They 
usually  try  to  control  fighting  by  manifesting  their  disapproval.  They  also 
have  learned  that  withholding  privileges  helps,  but  by  and  large  they  feel 
that  as  the  boys  stay  with  the  groups  these  problems  diminish  by  them- 
selves. They  have  more  difficulty  when  they  themselves  are  attacked  be- 
cause the  old  patterns  of  self-defense  come  into  conflict  with  their  newly 
found  professional  approach. 

The  groups  are  designed  to  improve  the  coping  skills  of  the  teenagers 
and  to  help  them  towards  more  positive  attitudes.  At  times  the  group 
seems  to  repeat  past  experiences  of  the  youngsters-for  instance,  when 


5  "Playing  the  Dozens"-the  act  of  talking  about  another  person's  parent  with  the 
intent  of  hurting  the  person's  feelings.  Foul  and  abusive  language  is  often  used.  "The 
dozens"-a  term  used  for  the  act  of  "playing  the  dozens."  Definition  from  "A  Diction- 
ary of  Local  Terms  and  Expressions"  edited  by  Mitchell,  Lonniej  Ph.D.  ("Definitions 
primarily  contributed  by  Baker's  Dozen  aides.) 

67 


they  visit  Washington  museums,  etc-  However,  one  finds  on  closer  exami- 
nation that  although  all  of  these  children  have  been  herded  through  oji 
educational  tours,  raiely  has  someone  discussed  it  all  with  them  in  terms 
they  could  understand  and  helped  them  with  it.  When  necessary  the  aides 
intercede  with  other  agencies  such  as  the  schools.  Contact  is  made  and 
maintained  with  the  young  people  even  when  they  leave  the  group.  The 
adolescents  sometimes  seek  intense  contact  with  the  aides  and  the  aides 
have  neither  discouraged  nor  encouraged  this,  but  accept  it.  One  little  girl 
used  to  show  up  at  an  aide's  apartment  on  Sunday  just  to  say  hello  and 
then  leave,  The  aides  lend  money  if  asked,  and  all  have  been  asked.  They 
try  a  variety  of  approaches  and  are  not  bound  by  tradition. 

The  aides  comment  that  the  predominant  motif  in  the  boys'  groups  is 
that  of  aggression  and  sex.  Sex  is  the  predominant  concern  for  the  adoles- 
cent girls'  groups,  All  the  aides  agreed  that  the  girls  in  this  neighborhood 
"get  wise"  too  young.  By  ages  11   to  12  they  are  too  seductive,  loo 
stimulated.  Many  of  the  girls  will  become  illegitimately  pregnant,  The 
aides  stress  that  this  tends  to  fmther  trap  the  girl  who  wishes  to  escape  the 
ghetto.  "Theft,  murder,  fornication,  desertion  are  so  much  a  part  of  their 
lives  that  they  become  indifferent  to  what  would  shock  other  people."6 
The  aides  see  in  their  group  members  the  patterns  of  impulsive  living  for 
the  moment,  and  they  try  to  help  their  group  members  find  other  ways  of 
living.  They  talk  to  their  groups.  One  aide,  herself  pregnant,  has  been 
asked  "how  it  feels  to  be  pregnant,"  All  of  the  women  aides  find  such 
issues  under  discussion,  and  they  meet  them  openly  and  honestly  and  Iry 
to  give  guidance.  They  are  in  close  contact  with  the  context  of  the  ques- 
tions and   can  answer  them  more  appropriately  than  someone   from 
another  background.  Although  the  planned  programs  are  developed  by  the 
teenagers  and  include  such  activities  as  movies,  parties,  cooking,  makeup, 
etc.,  depending  on  the  sex  of  the  group,  doubtless  the  informal  activity 
and  discussions  are  also  valuable. 

The  aides  try  to  foster  a  feeling  of  concern  among  their  group  members 
and  to  combat  the  feelings  of  helplessness,  isolation,  and  indifference  so 
common  among  their  group  members. 

Since  Baker's  Dozen  also  serves  as  a  training  placement  for  social  group 
and  caseworkers  and  psychologists,  the  aides'  intimate  knowledge  of  tins 
culture  is  passed  on  to  professionals  in  many  ways.  In  their  training  pro- 
gram, the  aides  have  developed  a  book  of  slang  vocabulary  phrases  com- 
mon to  the  area.  They  have  included  street  talk,  homosexual  jargon,  drug- 
addict  talk,  and  prison  terms.  Publication  is  being  considered  at  this  time, 
In  addition,  a  series  of  radio  programs  was  written  by  the  aides  and 
presented  over  a  period  of  months.  They  discussed  their  training  program, 
their  perspective  on  the  community,  their  ideas  on  why  children  mis- 
behave, and  other  topics.7 


6  "What  It  Means  To  Be  Really  Poor." 


Radio  program  series;  "The  Nonprofessional  Youth  in  the  Community."  Station 
WOL,  Washington,  D.C.,  November  1 965-February  1966 

68 


Research 

Research  data  are  being  collected  utilizing  the  self-reports  of  the  aides 
and  projective  measures  and  observations  by  others  of  the  aides  in  both 
experimental  and  natural  situations.  The  program  is  being  studied  in  terms 
of  such  issues  as  staff  roles  and  decision-making  proccduies.  Crises  are 
being  noted  and  followed  by  the  research  staff.  Records  are  being  kept  on 
attendance  of  both  aides  and  group  members.  Job  performance  lalings  on 
the  aides,  background  data  on  the  social  situation  of  the  aides,  their 
families,  etc.,  are  being  kept.  Periodic  evaluations  are  added  to  the  initial 
comprehensive  personal  evaluation.  The  aides  aie  asked  foi  indications  of 
their  self-image  and  their  self-esteem.  Their  patterns  of  behavior  (as  seen 
in  such  things  as  impulse  control),  their  levels  of  aspiration,  values, 
anxieties,  and  other  issues  are  being  noted  and  measured. 

Other  Issues 

The  entire  issue  undei  discussion— of  the  effectiveness  of  the  indigenous 
worker  due  to  his  close  understanding  of  his  group's  culture— leads  to  a 
discussion  as  to  whether  middle-class  people,  the  professionals,  can  them- 
selves be  effective  with  the  aides.  The  staff  report  that  this  is  entirely 
possible  but  that  certain  conditions  are  essential.  It  is  essential  that  the 
staff  understand  the  realities  of  life  in  the  slum.  They  must  undcistand  the 
obstacles  that  have  been  presented  the  aides.  They  must  understand  the 
ways  in  which  the  aides  have  not  been  included  in  the  mainstream  of 
opportunity.  They  must  understand  the  emphasis  placed  upon  money.  At 
Baker's  Dozen  the  aides  are  all  Negro;  the  staff  is  primarily  Negro.  The 
staff  and  aides  see  the  bonds  and  differences  not  primarily  in  terms  of 
color,  however,  but  in  terms  of  class  background.  All  staff,  regardless  of 
race,  must  be  able  to  tolerate  hostile  remarks  about  the  white  community. 
In  this  area  of  the  city  the  problems  faced  by  the  residents  arc  brought 
about  both  by  being  poor  and  being  Negro,  and  both  factors  have  to  be 
considered.  On  the  other  hand,  the  middle-class  professional  provides 
these  aides  with  a  glimpse  of  a  life  they  hadn't  really  seen  firsthand 
before.  One  aide  said  he  couldn't  wait  to  leave  "this  lousy  area"  and  that 
his  ambition  was  to  be  successful  enough  to  buy  a  "house  next  to 
Dr.  . .  .and  put  a  little  cast  iron  black  boy  out  front  and  paint  it 
white  .  . .,"  the  best  thing  of  all  being  he  would  know  it  would  still  be 
there  in  the  morning.  This  also  highlights  one  of  the  problems  of  the 
program.  The  job  requires  that  the  aides  not  move  out  of  the  area  for  the 
duration  of  the  program,  and  they  are  chafed  by  this  restriction.  They 
have  gotten  a  good  job  and  they  want  to  move  out.  They  do  accept  it  as  a 
realistic  requirement  but  vow  to  move  as  soon  as  possible.  Therefore  it  is 
reasonable  to  assume  that  they  will  leave  this  area.  It  has  been  an  interest- 
ing aspect,  and  one  now  being  studied,  that  these  aides  had  lived  in  the 
area  and  were  known  by  many  people.  They  have  provided  a  visible  model 
for  other  young  people  as  long  as  they  have  lived  in  the  area.  The  effect 
on  those  who  knew  them  and  on  the  area  is  being  assessed. 

69 


Although  many  issues  have  to  be  dealt  with  sensitively,  the  aides  hi 
been  found  to  have  a  far  higher  tolerance  for  frustration  than  predict 
With  guidance,  the  number  of  severe  disruptions  has  been  few.  The  nc 
verbal  nature  of  the  group  makes  it  difficult  for  staff.  The  aides  tend 
gieet  each  new  topic  with  silence.  They  also  tend  to  cast  the  staff  in  11 
role  of  boss.  Then,  too,  the  very  process  of  helping  people  to  a  new  vie 
of  themselves  makes  the  leader  vulnerable  to  their  new  strength,  ft 
instance,  the  aides  have  learned  the  power  of  group  activity  through  tlie, 
study  of  the  civil  rights  movement.  Now  they  are  aware  that  if  the 
become  seriously  dissatisfied  they  can  quit  en  masse  and  jeopardize  th< 
program  and  the  leader. 

The  aides  cite  money  as  a  major  asset  of  the  program,  The  importnnct 
to  them  of  the  opportunity  for  a  decent  job  has  been  tremendous.  They 
are  deeply  conceined  about  future  work.  They  question  whether  or  not 
they  will  actually  be  able  to  get  jobs  in  other  similar  settings.  They  still 
feel  as  though  the  system  is  closed  to  them.  Many  of  them  are  not  deeply 
committed  to  human  relations  work,  although  they  would  probably  con- 
tinue in  it  if  a  future  existed  for  them  there.  All  feel  the  program  has 
changed  their  lives.  They  are  strongly  attached  to  the  staff  in  the  program 
and  are  loyal  to  the  agency. 


Results 

The  most  impressive  finding  is  that  young  people  such  as  these  can  be 
trained  as  aides  and  can  do  the  work  successfully.  Although  levels  of 
efficiency  vary,  all  have  been  working  at  an  acceptable  level.  An  effective 
training  piogram  has  been  developed  and  has  been  found  to  motivate  and 
hold  the  young  people.  Despite  their  youth,  and  many  are  in  need  of 
employment  at  an  age  far  younger  than  most  professionals  reach  a  job, 
they  are  able  to  handle  the  responsibility.  The  aides  have  served  as  a 
bridge  between  the  professionals  and  the  people  being  served  and  have 
served  themselves  as  well. 

The  staff  reports  that  major  changes  seen  in  the  aides  can  be  accounted 
for  by  having  steady,  meaningful  employment  which  has  enabled  them  to 
support  themselves  and  to  stabilize  their  lives.  Marked  personality  change 
has  not  occurred,  but  social  adjustment  has  improved  markedly.  Both  the 
high-  and  low-risk  groups  performed  well  and,  with  the  exception  of  one 
boy  who  dropped  out  early  owing  to  trouble  with  the  police,  there  were 
no  essential  differences.  However,  many  of  these  people  have  histories  of 
difficulty  that  follow  them.  For  instance,  the  one  dropout  would  have 
returned,  but  because  he  was  in  prison  he  fell  behind  in  payments  set  by  a 
previous  paternity  suit  and  was  put  back  in  jail. 

Despite  the  fact  that  the  backgrounds  of  the  aides  were  similar  to  those 
of  their  clients,  they  seemed  to  see  the  problems  clearly  and  to  want  to 
help  the  kids  get  out  of  patterns  that  would  lead  to  trouble.  The  staff 
found  that  the  aides  could  cope  with  many  difficult  situations  and  that, 
with  the  supervision  provided  them,  they  could  perform  many  functions. 

70 


In  a  program  with  a  strong  rehabilitative  design  like  this,  one  has  to 
search  out  the  needs  of  the  aides  and  provide  opportunities  to  fill  these 
needs  during  the  period  of  training.  Staff  must  be  tolerant  and  capable. 
The  aides  say  that  their  neighborhood  is  slowly  getting  better.  It  is  ob- 
vious that  it  still  needs  improving.  The  mental  health  of  anyone  living  in 
such  an  area  is  inevitably  impaired.  The  awareness  that  much  needs  to  be 
done  has  not  always  sharpened  the  understanding  of  how  to  do  it.  The 
staff  at  Baker's  Dozen  and  the  Institute  for  Youth  Studies  ate  finding  a 
way. 

Research  Grant-  MH  14837 

Dates  of  Interviews:  Jan.  31  and  Feb.  3,7,8,  10,  and  17,  1967 

References. 

Denham,  W.,  Felsenfeld,  Naomi,  and  Walker,  W.  The  neighborhood  worker,  a  new 
resource  for  community  change.  A  monograph  on  training  and  utilization.  Institute 
for  Youth  Studies,  Howard  University,  Washington,  D.C.,  May  1966. 

Klein,  W,,  Denham,  W.,  MacLennan,  Beryce,  and  Fishman,  J.  Training  nonprofessiortal 
workers  for  human  services,  A  manual  of  organization  and  process.  Institute  for 
Youth  Studies,  Howard  University,  Washington,  D.C.,  May  1966. 

Klein,  W.,  Walker,  W,,  Levine,  Myrna,  MacLennan,  Beryce,  and  Fishman,  J.  Leadership 
in  (he  training  of  human  service  aides:  First  report  on  the  counseling  intern  pro- 
gram. Institute  for  Youth  Studies,  Howaid  University,  Washington,  D.C.,  1965-66. 

Mitchell,  L.  Psychotherapy  with  the  cultiually  and  economically  deprived  youth. 
Paper  read  at  annual  convention  of  the  Ameiican  Psychological  Association,  New 
York,  1966. 

Mitchell,  L.  Training  for  community  mental  health  aides  as  leaders  of  child  and  adoles- 
cent therapeutic  activity  groups.  Institute  for  Youth  Studies,  Ilowaid  Univeisity, 
Washington,  D.C.,  May,  1966. 

Mitchell,  L.  (Ed.)  A  dictionary  of  local  terms  and  expressions.  The  Baker's  Dozen 
Community  Mental  Health  Center  for  Adolescents;  Institute  for  Youth  Studies, 
Howard  University,  Washington,  D.C.,  1967. 

Mitchell,  L.,  and  Fishman,  J.  Mental  health  for  the  poor-the  use  of  trained  problem 
youth  in  a  neighborhood  treatment  program  for  children  and  adolescents.  Paper 
read  at  122d  annual  meeting  of  the  American  Psychiatric  Association,  Atlantic 
City,  May  1966. 

Training  for  new  careers.  President's  Committee  on  Juvenile  Delinquency  and  Youth 
Crime.  June  1965. 

"What  It  Means  To  Be  Really  Poor."  Bakei's  Dozen  Community  Mental  Health  Center 
aides  and  Mitchell,  L.  Mimeographed  paper,  1966. 


71 


Investigator. 

Metvin  Seeman,  Ph.D. 
University  of  California 
Los  Angeles,  Calif. 

Prepared  by 

Antoinette  Gattozzi 

Although  it  is  probably  true  that  there  never  has  been  a  human  com- 
munity without  its  critics,  the  case  against  modern  industrial  society  may 
be  unique  in  history  for  the  intellectual  sophistication  and  emotional 
appeal  of  its  arguments.  Distinguished  scholars  such  as  Arendt,  Marcusc, 
Fromnij  and  Mills  have  formulated  the  radical  critique  for  our  own  time, 
The  critique  has  an  illustrious  history,  moreover,  with  main  roots  in  the 
19th-century  writings  of  Durkheim,  Weber,  and  Marx.  The  works  in  this 
genre  now  form  a  more  or  less  cohesive  body  of  literature  known  as  mass 
society  theory,  The  mass  society  has  been  criticized  in  many  particulars, 
but  the  common  judgment  spanning  the  decades  is  that  a  mass  society  is, 
on  the  whole,  an  unhealthy  one.  Indeedj  mass  society  analysts  have  been 
aptly  called  the  pathologists  of  contemporary  industrial  societies. 

Alienation  is  the  dominant  theme  in  mass  society  analysis.  Alienation  is 
regarded  as  the  psychological  effect  generated  by  the  structural  forms  that 
define  a  mass  society  and,  in  turn,  it  is  considered  to  be  the  primary  cause 
of  a  multitude  of  personal  ills  and  social  evils.  The  pivotal  role  assigned  to 
the  alienation  concept  can  be  made  clear  by  outlining  the  central  thesis  of 
the  mass  society  literature.  The  thesis  is  essentially  composed  of  three 
elements-a  historically  oriented  view  of  contemporary  social  forms,  the 
concept  of  alienation,  and  judgments  about  the  quality  of  contemporary 
life. 

A  mass  society  is  defined  by  it?  structural  features,  which  are  the  forms 
inherent  in  the  historical  developments  of  democratization,  urbanization, 
and  industrialization.  It  is  a  society  in  which  most  major  institutions  are 
designed  to  deal  with  people  in  the  aggregate  without  distinguishing 
among  individuals  or  small  groups  of  individuals.  "Mass"  refers  not  to 
large  size  or  huge  numbers  per  se,  but  to  the  enlargement  of  the  scale  of 
social  forms.  Thus  national  governments,  nationwide  corporations,  trans- 
portation and  communication  systems  regularly  make  decisions  that  affect 
whole  populations,  Mass  culture,  mass  production,  mass  consumption  sup- 
plant the  natural  heterogeneity  of  the  people,  Pressures  for  technical 
efficiency  and  rational  control  lead  to  the  bureaucratization  of  organiza- 
tions: community  is  lost.  Geographical  mobility  displaces  stability.  Kin- 
ship ties  are  attenuated:  anonymity  and  impersonality  come  to  character- 
ize relations  among  people, 

72 


Mass  society  theorists  have  argued  that  alienation  is  fostered  among  the 
members  of  a  society  with  these  structural  features.  The  alienated  man 
suffers  incalculable  losses  of  many  personal  satisfactions  as  an  individual 
human  being.  He  is,  as  a  consequence,  especially  vulnerable  to  mental  and 
emotional  disturbances.  Further,  a  society  in  which  there  are  large  num- 
bers of  alienated  men  has  little  cohesiveness  and  organic  integrity.  A  mass 
society  may  be  beset,  for  example,  by  such  social  evils  as  widespread 
political  passivity,  ethnic  and  racial  prejudices,  and  wildcat  strikes  of  in 
dustrial  workers. 

This  capsule  statement  of  the  mass  society  theory  does  not  do  justice  to 
the  persuasiveness  of  the  formulations,  but  it  does  suggest  the  seriousness 
of  the  charges  made.  There  is  no  doubt  that  these  ideas  and  assertions, 
particularly  the  concept  of  alienation,  have  persisted  because  they  are 
intellectually  and  emotionally  seductive  to  many  thoughtful  people.  Yet 
the  literature  of  mass  society  theory  tends  to  be  discursive  and  rhetorical, 
and  the  use  of  the  crucial  alienation  concept  rather  more  exhortatory  than 
analytical.  As  a  result  Prof,  Melvin  Seeman  has  noted,  "the  debate  con- 
cerning alienation  has  often  remained  sterile,  however  valid  the  critique  of 
society  and  however  proper  and  humane  the  values  involved."  Professor 
Seeman  has  been  trying  to  clarify  the  terms  of  that  debate  and  to  derive 
some  specific  propositions  about  alienation  that  would  lend  themselves  to 
systematic  testing. 

The  task  began  in  1959  with  the  publication  of  a  conceptual  paper  in 
which  Professor  Seeman  suggested  that  the  various  connotations  of  aliena- 
tion could  be  distilled  into  five  related  but  separate  ideas.1  In  defining 
these  ideas,  he  drew  upon  the  social  learning  theory  of  Julian  B.  Rotter,  a 
theory  that  Professor  Seeman  thinks  has  much  in  common  with  the  aliena- 
tive  notion  of  powerlessness  and  its  consequences.  These  five  varieties  of 
alienation,  then,  were  defined  in  terms  of  an  individual's  expectations  and 
his  values. 

•  Powerlessness.  The  person  who  experiences  a  sense  of  powerlessness 
expects  that  forces  outside  himself  control  his  personal  and  social  rewards. 
He  has  little  expectancy  that  his  own  behavior  can  be  efficacious  in  gain- 
ing these  rewards. 

•  Meaninglessness.  The  individual  regards  social  affairs  as  incompre- 
hensible. Therefore,  he  has  little  expectancy  that  he  can  predict  the  out- 
come of  social  events. 

•  Normlessness,  The  individual  believes  that  he  is  not  bound  by  con- 
ventional standards  of  conduct  in  the  pursuit  of  his  goals,  Normlessness, 
on  the  contrary,  implies  a  high  expectancy  that  socially  unapproved 
means  must  be  used  to  achieve  these  goals. 

•  Value  isolation.   The   individual  rejects  the  values  of  society.  He 
assigns  low  value  to  the  goals  and  behaviors  that  are  highly  valued  by 
most  other  members  of  his  society. 


1  Since  that  time,  Professor  Seeman  has  added  a  sixth  form  of  alienation,  social 
isolation,  which  he  defines  as  an  individual's  low  expectancy  for  social  acceptance,  as 
reflected,  for  example,  in  the  feelings  of  loneliness  and  exclusion  experienced  by 
members  of  minority  groups. 

73 


a  Self-estrangement.  While  this  idea  has  taken  a  number  of  definitional 
forms,  the  one  in  which  it  is  perhaps  most  easily  grasped  is  this:  To  be 
self-estranged  is  to  be  engaged  in  activities  that  are  not  rewarding  in  them- 
selves. This  is  self-estrangement  in  the  classic  Marxian  sense  of  alienated 
labor.  In  social  learning  terms,  the  self-estranged  person  is  continuously 
engaged  in  activities  he  does  not  value  highly. 

Professor  Seeman  has  noted  that  the  obverse  of  these  alienative  forms- 
order  and  trust,  consensus  and  commitment,  integrity  and  engagement- 
represent  humanistic  values  that  are  highly  esteemed  by  democratic  soci- 
eties. Thus,  to  the  extent  alienation  is  engendered  in  a  society  such  as  outs 
in  the  United  States,  it  exerts  profoundly  detrimental  effects  on  individual 
lives  and  threatens  to  make  a  mockery  of  our  most  cherished  values,  The 
crucial  questions,  of  course,  are  whether  or  not  the  social  forms  of  our 
modern  industrial  society  really  do  spawn  alienation  among  individual 
Americans  and,  if  so,  whether  or  not  alienation  has  the  behavioral  conse- 
quences that  mass  society  theory  would  predict. 

It  is  the  second  question  that  has  chiefly  interested  Professor  Seeman. 
In  a  series  of  empirical  studies  carried  out  over  the  last  10  years,  lie  has 
looked  at  two  forms  of  alienation,  powerlessness  and  self-estrangement, 
and  has  sought  to  discover  whether  an  individual's  level  of  alienation  is 
related  to  his  behavior  in  certain  circumscribed  areas. 


Powerlessness  and  Learning 

As  noted  above,  Professor  Seeman  utilized  certain  constructs  from  a 
theory  of  social  learning  in  his  explication  of  alienation .  The  similarities 
between  this  theory  and  the  mass  society  view  of  the  behavioral  conse- 
quences of  an  individual's  powerlessness  are  quite  striking. 

The  social  learning  theory  formulated  by  J.  B.  Rotter  holds  that  a 
person's  behavior  is  significantly  influenced  by  two  factors  and  by  the 
situation  in  which  they  occur  or  are  embedded,  It  depends  on  his  expect- 
ancy that  the  behavior  will  lead  to  a  successful  outcome  and  on  the  value 
he  places  on  that  outcome.  Moreover,  the  theory  distinguishes  between 
internal  and  external  control  of  rewards.  Dr.  Rotter  and  his  associates 
have  postulated  that,  in  laboratory  experiments  in  learning,  subjects  will 
not  do  as  well  when  they  perceive  their  success  as  being  dependent  on 
chance  or  luck  or  on  the  experimenter's  control  of  the  situation  as  when 
they  believe  that  their  own  skill  can  decide  the  outcome,  (A  number  of 
investigations  have  since  shown  that  this  is  indeed  true.)  Similarly,  mass 
society  analysts  have  contended  that  the  individual  living  in  a  contemp- 
orary industrial  society,  believing  that  he  is  powerless  to  determine  the 
successful  outcome  of  his  social  behaviors,  turns  away  from  political  activ- 
ities; his  sense  of  powerlessness  makes  him  indifferent  to  and  a  poor 
learner  of  political  information  and  of  other  knowledge  relevant  to  his 
social  functioning. 

74 


"Thus,"  Professor  Seeman  has  commented,  "the  idea  of  powerlessness 
extends  downward,  as  it  were,  in  its  potential  for  reorganizing  the  rela- 
tively 'microscopic'  studies  of  laboratory  learning  .  .  .  But  the  idea  of 
powerlessness  also  extends  upward  in  its  significance,  being  an  integral 
element  in  sociological  descriptions  of  'macroscopic'  concerns:  The  occur- 
rence of  mass  movements,  the  conditions  of  political  democracy,  and  the 
like." 

The  first  of  a  series  of  controlled  studies  testing  the  relation  between 
powerlessness  and  learning  was  conducted  by  Professor  Seeman  and  a 
colleague,  John  W.  Evans,  among  the  patients  in  10  tuberculosis  sanitari- 
ums in  Ohio.  Their  hypothesis  was  that  a  patient's  sense  of  powerlessness 
influenced  what  he  had  learned  about  tubeiculosis.  They  predicted  that 
high  poweilessness  would  be  associated  with  poor  learning. 

A  total  of  86  white  male  subjects  were  selected  from  a  much  larger 
sample  and  grouped  into  43  pairs  closely  matched  forage,  income,  educa- 
tion, length  of  hospitalization,  estimated  discharge  time  and,  most  impor- 
tant, hospital  experience— each  individual  in  a  pair  lived  on  the  same  ward 
of  a  hospital  and  was  exposed  to  the  same  routines  and  staff  care.  They 
differed,  of  course,  in  their  positions  on  the  powerlessness  scale. 

The  scale  consisted  of  forced-choice  items  designed  to  reveal  an  indi- 
vidual's expectancies  for  personal  control.2  Most  items  referred  to  socio- 
political expectancies.  For  example,  the  patients  were  asked  to  choose,  on 
the  basis  of  personal  belief,  between  these  two  statements:  'The  average 
citizen  can  have  an  influence  on  the  way  the  government  is  run,"  and 
"This  world  is  run  by  the  few  people  in  power  and  there  is  not  much  the 
little  guy  can  do  about  it."  The  scores  of  the  total  sample  were  divided  at 
the  mean  of  the  distribution  into  "low"  and  "high"  alienation.  Each  pair 
of  matched  patients,  then,  included  an  "unalienated"  and  an  "alienated" 
person. 

The  investigators  also  needed  a  measure  of  each  patient's  knowledge 
about  tuberculosis.  This  they  obtained  by  a  standard  true-false  informa- 
tion test  based  on  one  used  by  the  National  Tuberculosis  Association. 
Then,  to  get  an  idea  of  the  extent  to  which  a  patient's  objective  knowl- 
edge was  manifested  in  his  ward  behavior,  the  staff  was  asked  a  number  of 
questions  about  the  behavior  of  individual  patients.  Two  questions  were 
pertinent:  How  good  is  this  patient's  understanding  of  his  illness,  and  of 
the  disease  generally?  This  gave  the  investigators  a  measure  of  what  they 
called  reputed  knowledge.  Finally,  they  obtained  from  each  patient  a 
measure  of  his  subjective  knowledge;  that  is,  an  indication  of  how  satisfied 
he  felt  with  the  knowledge  about  tuberculosis  that  he  possessed.  These 
latter  two  measures  were  taken  solely  to  help  shed  light  on  the  re- 
searcher's central  inquiry,  which  concerned  the  postulated  relationship 
between  an  individual's  expectancies  for  control  and  his  objective  knowl- 
edge about  an  event— tuberculosis— that  strongly  affected  his  life  career. 


2  The  alienation  measure  used  in  this  and  the  other  studies  reported  here  was 
adapted  from  the  I— E  Scale  (internal-external  control)  developed  by  Professor  J.  B. 
Rotter  and  the  late  Professor  Shephard  Liverant  at  the  Ohio  State  University.  The  I~E 
Scale  has  been  extensively  tested  and  refined  and  shows  satisfactory  reliability. 

75 


The  results  confirmed  the  prediction:  Alienated  patients  scored  lower 
on  the  information  test  than  did  unalienated  patients.  The  difference 
between  the  two  groups  was  small  but  statistically  significant.  The  investi- 
gators showed  that  the  difference  in  knowledge  about  tuberculosis  did  not 
reflect  any  consistent  differences  in  intelligence  among  individuals  in  Uic 
two  groups. 

Staff  evaluation  of  a  subject's  knowledge,  so-called  reputed  knowledge, 
was  in  line  with  the  main  finding.  Patients  high  in  alienation  were  judged 
by  the  staff  to  be  less  well  informed  about  tuberculosis  than  palicrtls 
relatively  lower  in  alienation.  No  significant  coirelation  was  found  be- 
tween subjective  knowledge  and  alienation,  but  an  inteiesting  interaction 
was  discovered  between  ward  stratification  and  alienation.  The  iclalivcly 
more  controlled  environments  (controlled  by  the  physician  in  clungo) 
drew  fewer  responses  indicating  dissatisfaction  fiom  the  unalienated  than 
from  the  alienated  patients.  The  researchers  had  predicted  that  the  oppo- 
site would  be  the  case-that  those  who  felt  a  greater  mastery  would  resent 
the  tight  control  exerted  in  the  highly  stratified  ward.  They  offered  one 
possible  interpretation  of  this  finding,  A  highly  stratified  environment  is 
not  congenial  to  the  transmission  of  knowledge  and  it  could  be,  (liey 
suggested,  that  on  such  wards  the  alienated  patients  actively  sought  m\ 
gained  more  knowledge  than  alienated  patients  were  inclined  to  do,  In  a 
less  controlled  ward  setting,  knowledge  may  be  acquired  more  passively, 
and  thus  be  equally  available  to  those  willing  to  seek  it  (the  unalienated) 
and  those  for  whom  knowledge  presumably  has  little  value  and,  therefore, 
is  not  worth  any  effort  to  attain  (the  alienated).  This  interpretation  was 
modestly  substantiated  when  the  investigators  compared  objective  knowl- 
edge scores  of  the  unalienated  and  alienated  groups  on  the  two  different 
kinds  of  wards.  There  was  less  difference  between  the  group  scores  if 
patients  resided  on  a  low-stratification  ward  than  if  they  lived  on  a  highly 
stratified  ward. 

The  results  of  this  study,  then,  provided  a  reasonably  satisfactory 
demonstration  of  an  association  between  powerlessness  and  learning.  At 
least  two  questions  were  left  open,  however.  Was  the  relatively  poor  learn- 
ing shown  by  the  more  alienated  patients  the  product  of  their  greater 
sense  of  powerlessness  or  did  their  powerlessness  come  from  possessing 
little  knowledge?  Second,  information  from  only  one  domain  of  informa- 
tion was  tested-the  control-relevant  domain  of  tuberculosis  in  forma  (ion; 
would  the  more  alienated  patients  have  made  an  equally  poor  showing  in 
any  other  area  of  knowledge,  which  might  suggest  a  general  withdrawn!  of 
their  interest  in  learning?  The  design  of  Professor  Seeman's  next  study 
provided  ways  to  look  into  these  questions. 

The  next  study  was  carried  out  among  the  young  male  inmates  of  an 
Ohio  reformatory.  The  choice  of  setting  was  particularly  apt,  "It  is  pos- 
sible to  conceive  of  the  reformatory  and  its  associated  training  apparatus 
as  a  vast  learning  mechanism,"  Professor  Seeman  pointed  out,  "but  one  to 
which  the  essential  features  of  powerlessness  dominate  institutional  life- 
where,  for  example,  paroles  are  denied  and  inmates  left  in  ignorance  of 

76 


the  reasons,  and  where  the  inmate  culture  is  a  response  to  the  more  or  less 
total  threat  to  personal  control." 

The  alienation  scale  used  in  this  study  was  like  the  one  used  with  the 
tuberculosis  patients.  Once  again  the  scores  were  divided  at  the  mean  of 
distribution  so  that  the  subjects  could  be  described  as  high  or  low  in 
alienation.  The  tests  of  learning  weie  quite  different.  This  time  Professor 
Seeman  tested  for  new  learning  rather  than  measure  prior  knowledge.  To 
do  so,  the  group  of  85  men  (none  having  less  than  an  IQ  of  100  or  a 
ninth-grade  education)  was  presented  with  24  items  of  information  about 
correction.  A  third  of  these  items  dealt  with  the  reformatory ,  a  third  were 
concerned  with  parole  matters,  and  a  third  dealt  with  long-iange  oppor- 
tunities. The  items  were  compiled  from  documents  not  readily  available  to 
the  men  and  presumably  represented  new  information. 

The  men  were  presented  with  this  material  twice  in  one  session.  In 
order  to  insure  that  each  item  would  be  read,  the  investigator  first  asked 
them  to  mark  off  how  interested  they  were  in  each  item,  moments  after 
this  task  was  completed,  the  men  were  given  the  items  arranged  in  a 
multiple-choice-test  format  which  they  were  asked  to  complete.  The  latter 
constituted  their  learning  scores,  for  it  tested  how  much  of  the  material 
they  had  retained. 

The  investigator  found  that  men  low  in  alienation  achieved  significantly 
better  recall  of  the  parole  items  than  did  men  high  in  alienation.  There 
was  no  statistically  significant  difference  between  the  alienation  groups  in 
the  recall  of  the  two  other  kinds  of  information. 

Professor  Seeman  also  made  an  estimate  of  the  value  each  man  placed 
on  the  conventional  norms  of  the  reformatory  by  compiling  the  number 
of  merit  commendations  each  had  earned  from  prison  authorities.  Dif- 
ferences in  powerlessness  were  found  to  be  unrelated  to  learning  among 
inmates  who  had  earned  no  merits,  but  high  or  low  powerlessness  made  a 
significant  difference  in  learning  among  men  who  had  earned  one  or  more 
merits.  The  highest  correlation  between  alienation  and  the  learning  of 
parole  items  appeared  when  low  powerlessness  was  accompanied  by  a 
degree  of  commitment  to  the  values  of  rehabilitation  (as  symbolized  by 
the  earning  of  merits).  Thus,  as  Rotter's  social  learning  theory  would 
suggest,  the  combination  of  high  expectation  for  personal  control  and 
high  evaluation  of  the  goals  in  question  was  most  revealing  of  the  associa- 
tion between  alienation  and  learning. 

Merit  commendations  were  an  indication  of  the  men's  behavior  in  the 
reformatory;  Professor  Seeman  also  obtained  some  indication  of  their 
behavior  outside  the  reformatory,  prior  to  confinement.  Although  age  and 
IQ  were  found  to  be  unrelated  to  learning  scores,  continuation  beyond  the 
ninth  grade  and  achievement  relative  to  capacity  were  relevant.  A  man's 
willingness  to  stay  in  school  and  to  achieve  in  accordance  with  his  capac- 
ity correlated  well  with  both  low  powerlessness  and  good  learning  of 
parole  material.  "The  most  interesting  feature  of  these  results,"  Professor 
Seeman  commented,  "lies  in  the  fact  that  the  inmate's  learning  of 
corrections-relevant  material  (the  parole  information)  is  related  not  only 

77 


to  his  generalized  expectancies  for  control  but  to  his  behavior-both  out- 
side the  prison  and  inside  it- which  presumably  reflects  such  expectan- 
cies." 

This  investigation  went  a  long  way  towards  claiifying  the  relations  be- 
tween behavior  and  alienation  in  the  powerlessness  sense  of  low  expect- 
ancy for  control.  It  yielded  a  clear  demonstration  of  an  association  be- 
tween learning  and  alienation.  More,  the  findings  suggested  some  limits  of 
this  association-it  appeals  only  in  the  learning  of  control-relevant 
information-and  revealed  the  fact  that  some  behavioral  concomitants  of 
alienation  may  be  found  outside  the  sphere  of  learning.  These  two  impor- 
tant additions  to  the  body  of  evidence  about  alienation  were  explored  in 
depth  in  the  third  study  of  this  series.  The  study  was  made  in  Sweden; 
Professor  Seeman  carried  out  a  number  of  related  investigations  in 
Sweden,  which  gave  him  an  opportunity  to  examine  the  cross-cultural 
validity  of  alienation  effects  on  learning  and  other  behaviors.  The  study  to 
be  described  next  was  conducted  among  some  300  students  .it  Lund 
University;  the  other  studies  wil!  be  discussed  below  in  different  contexts. 

The  basic  design  was  similar  to  that  of  the  other  investigations.  Stu- 
dents were  judged  to  be  high  or  low  in  alienation  on  the  basis  of  tlicir 
scores  on  the  powerlessness  scale,  and  each  student's  knowledge  in  one  or 
the  other  of  two  domains  of  information  was  tested.  One  was  the  domain 
of  cultural  knowledge  and  the  otliei  was  that  of  nuclear  weapons  (for  ii 
small  subsample,  political  information  was  also  tested).  All  tests  weic 
equal  ia  difficulty.  Professor  Seeman  predicted  that  high  alienation 
would  be  associated  with  poor  scores  on  the  nuclear  (or  political)  test,  but 
alienation  would  be  irrelevant  to  scores  on  the  cultural  test.  In  other 
words,  he  hypothesized  that  alienation  would  affect  learning  differential- 
ly, as  was  suggested  by  the  results  of  the  reformatory  study,  and  that  the 
acquisition  of  control -relevant  information  is  the  specific  learning  most 
affected  by  one's  relative  sense  of  mastery  or  powerlessness. 

Another  alienation  proposition  was  tested  in  this  work.  Prior  to  the 
initiation  of  the  study,  each  student  had  completed  a  short  veision  of  tlie 
alienation  scale,  and  these  scores  were  in  hand  when  the  study  proper  got 
under  way.  For  the  study  proper,  then,  most  students  received  a  cultural 
or  a  nuclear  test  and  a  long  version  of  the  alienation  scale  by  mail,  along 
with  a  covering  letter  and  return  envelope.  Professor  Seeman  was  inter- 
ested in  those  who  delayed  returning  the  tests  or  failed  to  do  so  alto- 
gether, that  is,  those  who  showed  avoidance  behavior.  He  predicted  that 
high  alienation  would  be  associated  with  late  or  nonreturns  among  the 
group  that  got  the  nuclear  test  but  that  no  such  association  would  appear 
among  those  getting  the  cultural  test.  This,  he  suggested,  would  allow  for 
a  "mierodemonstration"  of  the  proposition  that  powerlessness  leads  to 
avoidance  behavior™  "micro-"  because  what  was  involved  was  "the  small 
world  of  everyday  tasks,  like  responding  to  an  inquiry  from  the  univer- 
sity." 

The  results  concerning  alienation  and  control -relevant  learning  con- 
firmed the  prediction:  High  alienation,  especially  for  women,  correlated 

78 


with  poor  knowledge  of  nuclear  weapons  or  political  affairs,  but  alien- 
ation, high  or  low,  showed  no  associations  with  cultural  knowledge,  poor 
or  good. 

The  data  did  not  leveal  so  clearcut  an  answer  to  the  question  about 
avoidance  behavior  and  alienation.  The  results  tended  to  confirm  the 
prediction— that  is,  alienated  students  were  slower  to  return  and  more 
often  failed  to  return  the  packet  of  tests  if  they  had  gotten  a  nuclear  test 
than  if  they  had  received  a  cultural  test— but  did  not  reach  statistical 
significance.  There  was,  in  fact,  an  astonishingly  high  rate  of  return  (85 
percent)  before  followup  letters  were  sent  out,  which  might  be  attribut- 
able to  the  fact  that  in  Sweden  theie  are  strong,  though  informal, 
pressures  to  cooperate  in  social  research. 

This  study  showed  that  the  proposition  concerning  the  effects  of 
powerlessness  on  differential  learning  holds  in  the  domain  of  sociopolitical 
information  and  that  it  is  valid  for  college-age  students  of  another  culture. 
Taken  together,  the  three  studies  of  alienation  and  powerlessness  convinc- 
ingly demonstrated  the  applicability  of  this  proposition  to  a  range  of 
information  domains  and  to  different  populations  of  people.  In  so  doing, 
Professor  Seeman's  work  greatly  strengthened  the  empirical  basis  of  the 
proposition's  credibility  and  its  usefulness  in  other  social  research.  For 
example,  its  applicability  to  a  sensitive  and  vital  area  of  American  life,  the 
education  of  young  children,  was  recently  demonstrated  by  James  S.  Cole- 
man  and  co-authors  in  their  report,  Equality  of  Educational  Opportun- 
ity,* Professor  Seeman  has  cited  the  relevant  passage:  "*  *  *  a  pupil 
factor  which  appears  to  have  a  stronger  relationship  to  achievement  than 
do  all  the  'school'  factors  taken  together  is  the  extent  to  which  an  individ- 
ual feels  that  he  has  some  control  over  his  destiny."  To  the  degree  the 
proposition  linking  high  alienation  and  poor  learning  is  valid  in  other  areas 
as  well,  it  bears  important  implications  for  those  trying  to  increase  public 
understanding  of  international  political  issues,  say,  or  those  responsible 
for  public  health  information  campaigns  about  alcoholism,  for  instance,  or 
the  value  of  prenatal  care. 


The  Role  of  Organizational  Ties 

Mass  society  theorists  have  made  an  important  recommendation  in  their 
writings.  They  have  repeatedly  asserted  that  organizational  ties  must  be 
established  by  the  individual  in  order  for  him  to  have  an  effective  media- 
tor vis-a-vis  the  mass-scale  institutions  that  surround  him.  This  theme  is 
sounded  throughout  the  mass  society  literature,  from  Durkheiin  to  Mills; 
although  Mills,  in  The  Power  Elite,  expressed  doubts  about  the  efficacy  of 
mediating  organizations  in  our  time  because  they,  too,  as  he  saw  them, 
had  begun  to  assume  mass-scale  qualities. 


'U.S.  Government  Printing  Office.  Washington,  D.C.  1966. 

79 


Perhaps  the  most  familiar  example  of  a  mediating  organizatioi 
labor  union,  and  mass  society  theory  would  predict  that  union  mci 
ship  mitigates  a  worker's  sense  of  alienation.  To  test  this,  Professor 
man  and  a  colleague,  Arthui  G.  Neal,  formulated  a  concrete  hypot 
that  could  be  examined  in  an  empirical  manner.  The  hypothesis: 
expected  members  of  a  work-based  formal  organization  to  exhibit 
powerlessness  than  individuals  without  an  organization  to  speak  for  t 
in  the  crucial  area  of  occupation," 

The  setting  for  the  study  designed  to  examine  this  hypothesis 
Columbus,  Ohio,  where  the  investigators  assembled  a  random  samph 
adult  male  subjects  from  the  city  directory.  Mail  questionnaires  were  u 
to  gather  necessary  information  about  each  man's  union  membership  i 
his  sense  of  powerlessness.  In  addition,  each  was  asked  to  answer  qi 
tions  about  his  experience  of  occupational  mobility  and  to  complex 
scale  designed  to  determine  his  attitudes  towards  mobility.  These  lal 
measures  were  needed,  the  researchers  noted,  because  several  studies  1; 
shown  mobility  and  mastery  (the  obverse  of  powerlessness)  to  tie  relate 
Finally,  subjects  also  received  the  well-known  anomie  scale,  developed  i 
Leo  Srole,  which  can  be  interpreted  as  measuring  generalized  despair,  T! 
was  included  because  the  investigators  wanted  to  find  out  if  nonmciubt 
ship  was  associated  with  other  forms  of  alienation  in  addition  to  powc 
lessness. 

Through  the  use  of  personal  interviews  done  after  the  returns  were  if 
the  investigators  checked  the  possibility  that  more  alienated  than  uiutHcr 
ated  men  would  have  delayed  or  not  bothered  at  all  to  comply  with  tli 
mailed  requests.  They  found  this  was  not  the  case:  Neither  late  return  no 
nonreturn  correlated  with  high  alienation.  The  investigators  received  re 
plies  from  slightly  more  than  600  men  (57  percent  of  the  original  sample), 
The  occupational  statuses  of  the  respondents  ranged  from  high-level  exec- 
utives to  unemployed  manual  workers.  Analyses  of  the  data  yielded  (he 
following  results. 

First,  the  basic  prediction  was  confirmed.  Compared  to  the  group  of 
organized  workers,  the  group  of  unorganized  workers  felt  themselves  to  be 
more  powerless.  This  finding  held  when  the  variables  of  occupational 
status  and  income  were  controlled.  When  the  mobility  variables  were 
taken  into  account,  on  the  other  hand,  the  investigators  did  find  some 
interesting  interactions. 

When  the  groups  of  organized  and  unorganized  men  were  separated  into 
manual  and  nonmanual  workers,  the  investigators  found  the  same  strong 
association  between  high  powerlessness  and  nonmembership  among  (he 
manual  workers,  regardless  of  mobility  history  or  attitude.  The  picture 
was  different  among  non-manual  workers,  For  this  group,  a  mobility  atti- 
tude of  nonstriving  reversed  the  effect.  Thus,  among  the  mobility-oriented 
white-collar  workers,  the  unorganized  were  higher  in  powerlessness  titan 
the  organized  (as  expected),  but  it  was  the  organized  workers  who  were 
higher  in  powerlessness  than  the  unorganized  in  the  group  of  nonstriving 
white-collar  workers.  These  data  enabled  the  researchers  to  add  a  useful 

80 


lefinement  to  the  thesis  about  organizational  ties:  It  is  particularly  appli- 
cable to  woikers  committed  to  mobility. 

Second,  no  consistent  relation  emerged  between  membership  or  non- 
membership  and  anomie,  although  unorganized  workers  tended  to  be  only 
slightly  higher  in  anomie  than  organized  workers.  By  implication,  anomie 
and  powerlessness  were  not  found  to  be  strongly  and  systematically  re- 
lated either.  This  negative  evidence  was  welcome  on  two  counts.  First,  it 
supported  the  investigators'  assumptions  that  their  hypothesis  was  a  rela- 
tively specific  one-i.e.,  membership  or  nonmembership  was  linked  to  the 
powerlessness  form  of  alienation— and  second,  it  was  suggested  that  pow- 
erlessness, as  they  defined  and  measured  it,  was  a  satisfactorily  specific 
form  of  alienation  that  excluded  more  generalized  feelings  of  hopeless- 
ness, 

Although  low  powerlessness  was  clearly  shown  to  be  associated  with 
organizational  membership,  the  fact  by  itself  does  not  support  the  mass 
society  thesis  regarding  mediating  organizations.  One  may  wonder,  as  the 
investigators  did,  whether  membership  leads  to  low  alienation  or  vice 
versa.  The  researchers  were  able  to  make  a  start  toward  the  answer  by 
obtaining  data  on  the  union  situation  (open  or  closed  shop,  maintenance- 
of-membership  contracts,  and  the  like)  in  the  firms  where  their  respond- 
ents worked.  While  conceding  that  their  findings  had  to  be  regarded  as 
only  tentative,  they  concluded  that  both  options  were  valid.  In  other 
words,  the  mass  society  argument  about  membership  ameliorating  the 
alienative  effects  of  social  structures  was  not  inconsistent  with  the  evi- 
dence; alternately,  the  data  also  provided  support  for  the  interpretation 
that  workers  low  in  alienation  may  be  motivated,  by  virtue  of  their  expec- 
tancy for  personal  control,  to  join  organizations  which  are  vehicles  for  the 
exercise  of  control  in  the  work  sphere. 

The  proposition  concerning  the  ameliorative  effects  of  organizational 
tics  was  directly  tested  in  one  other  population.  This  was  a  large  random 
sample,  some  550  men,  of  the  work  force  of  Malmo,  Sweden.  Professor 
Seeman  worked  intensively  with  this  group;  in  addition  to  the  data  re- 
quired for  the  mediation  thesis,  he  gathered  many  other  kinds  of  informa- 
tion pertaining  to  alienation  and  its  putative  effects  on  behavior  and  atti- 
tudes, Much  of  what  he  discovered  will  be  described  below  in  the  section 
on  alienated  labor.  Two  aspects  of  this  omnibus  Swedish  work  are  relevant 
at  this  point  of  the  report,  the  evidence  concerning  the  mediation  thesis 
and  the  data  demonstrating  anew,  in  the  context  of  the  mediation  thesis, 
the  relation  between  powerlessness  and  learning. 

In  Malmo',  as  in  Columbus,  high  powerlessness  and  poor  knowledge  of 
political  affairs  were  found  to  be  associated  in  the  group  of  unorganized 
workers,  manual  and  nonmanual;  there  was  no  such  association  to  be  seen 
among  the  organized  workers.  The  same  pattern  held  when  such  pertinent 
variables  as  education,  income,  and  occupational  prestige  were  controlled. 
Moreover,  there  was  a  modest  connection  between  degree  of  involvement 
and  both  powerlessness  and  learning:  Workers  who  were  little  involved  in 
their  organizations  scored  higher  in  powerlessness  and  lower  in  political 
knowledge  than  did  those  who  were  more  involved. 

81 


Thus,  in  two  dispaiate  cultural  settings,  men  who  were  members  of  a 
work  organization  experienced  less  personal  powerlessness  In  the  socio- 
political arena  than  did  their  unorganized  fellow  workers.  The  evidence 
gathered  in  the  two  studies  was  consistent  with  the  mass  society  thesis 
assigning  a  meliorative  role  to  such  oiganizations.  More  investigations  arc 
needed,  however,  to  determine  how  much  a  worker's  sense  of  powerless- 
ness  is  minimized  by  his  membership  in  a  woik  organization  as  opposed  to 
how  much  his  sense  of  his  ability  to  influence  conditions  that  affect  him 
leads  him  to  join  the  organization  in  the  first  place. 


Alienation  in  Work 

The  world  of  work  occupies  a  prominent  place  in  mass  society  theory, 
which  devotes  much  attention  to  alienated  labor.  An  alienated  worker  is 
defined  as  one  who  does  work  that  is  not  intrinsically  rewarding  to  liim. 
He  does  it  because  he  feels  he  must,  not  because  he  gains  personal  satisfac- 
tion in  the  activity.  To  engage  in  such  work  regularly,  then,  is  to  experi- 
ence a  variety  of  self-estrangement.  Mass  society  and  Marxian  theorists 
have  contended  that  the  existence  of  an  alienated  labor  force  is  itself 
responsible  for  a  plethora  of  social  problems.  The  deleterious  effects  arc 
seen  to  be  so  pervasive  and  profound,  in  fact,  that  Marx  and  others  have 
argued  that  many  of  the  social  ills  of  modern  industrial  societies  could  be 
made  to  disappear  if  only  the  alienation  of  laboi  were  ended. 

As  expressed  in  the  mass  society  literature,  the  consequences  of  alien- 
ated labor  include  attitudinal  effects  such  as  powerlessness  and  norniless- 
ness  as  well  as  behavioral  effects  such  as  minimal  political  participation, 
racial  and  ethnic  hostility,  and  the  substitution  of  extrinsic  goals  (job 
status,  for  example)  for  unattainable  intrinsic  satisfactions.  Professor  See- 
man  has  characterized  this  theme  of  the  mass  society  literatuie  as  tlic 
generalization  thesis.  He  has  begun  to  test  its  validity  in  separate  investiga- 
tions carried  out  in  Sweden,  France,  and  the  United  States.  The  Swedish 
study  has  been  completed  and  it  can  be  reported  in  full. 

A  random  sample  of  the  male  work  force  of  Malmti  constituted  tlie 
study  population.  Data  were  obtained  through  personal  interviews  witli 
the  workers.  The  basic  measure-whether  or  not  a  worker  felt  alienated 
from  his  work-was  obtained  by  reference  to  a  work  alienation  index, 
which  was  developed  by  factor  analysis  of  the  responses  to  pertinent 
questions.  The  scale  finally  developed  by  this  method  was  composed  of 
seven  items,  for  example,  "Is  your  job  too  simple  to  bring  out  your  best 
abilities,  or  not?"  The  investigator  also  obtained  measures  of  powerless- 
ness,  ethnic  and  racical  prejudice,  anomie,  political  knowledge,  orienta- 
tion to  experts,  and  mobility  orientation.  According  to  the  mass  society 
generalization  thesis,  a  man  who  is  alienated  from  his  work  feels  power- 
less,  hostile  to  "others,"  and  anomie;  he  is  rather  ignorant  of  political 
affairs,  tending  to  leave  them  in  the  hands  of  experts,  and  strives  for  the 
extrinsic  rewards  that  status  confers. 

82 


Reasonable  though  this  thesis  may  sound,  the  analyses  of  the  data 
yielded  very  little  support  for  it.  Work  alienation  was  not  found  to  be 
related  to  any  of  the  theoretical  outcomes  in  a  statistically  significant 
manner,  although  some  of  the  outcomes  did  relate  to  one  anothei-for 
example,  high  powerlessness  was  associated  with  scanty  political  knowl- 
edge and  high  expert  orientation  and  with  racial  hostility.  Even  when 
variables  such  as  age,  income,  occupation,  and  the  like  were  controlled,  no 
clear-cut  associations  with  work  alienation  emerged.  Further,  when  con- 
sidering solely  the  question  of  how  much  control  a  worker  felt  he  excited 
in  his  work  process-supposedly  the  quintessential  condition  of  alienated 
labor— the  associations  to  the  postulated  effects  were  similarly  minimal. 
The  conclusion  to  be  drawn  from  these  results,  then,  was  that  the  attitudi- 
nal  and  behavioral  consequences  repeatedly  attiibuted  to  alienation  in 
work  simply  do  not  reflect  the  reality  as  it  exists  in  Sweden. 

An  obvious  question  that  arises  is  whether  Sweden,  after  all,  should  not 
be  considered  an  exceptional  case  because  of  its  homogeneous  population, 
its  long  history  of  peace  and  of  social  and  economic  stability.  To  answer 
this  question,  Professor  Seeman  undertook  to  perform  essentially  similai 
studies  of  alienation  in  work  in  France  and  the  United  States.  These,  too, 
are  highly  industrialized  societies,  but  their  political  and  social  histories 
differ  markedly  from  Sweden's  and  are  different  from  one  another  as  well. 

Results  from  these  two  studies  are  not  yet  available.  Preliminary  analy- 
ses suggest,  however,  that  neither  the  French  noi  the  American  situation 
differs  from  the  Swedish  in  terms  of  the  consequences  of  alienated  labor. 
On  the  other  hand,  it  appears  that  the  relation  between  high  powerlessness 
and  low  political  knowledge  will  be  found  in  both  the  French  and  Ameri- 
can populations,  as  will  additional  validation  of  the  mediation  thesis.  (The 
study  in  Los  Angeles  should  also  help  illuminate  the  uniquely  American 
racial  situation,  In  addition  to  interviewing  some  500  adult  male  white 
subjects,  the  investigator  assembled  a  subsample  of  some  270  Negro 
workers.  It  should  be  instructive  to  see  how  the  data  collected  from  the 
white  workers  compare  to  those  gathered  in  the  Negro  subgioup.)  When 
the  results  from  all  three  countries  are  available,  the  generalization  thesis 
of  mass  society  theory  will  have  received  a  substantial  test. 

Even  if  alienated  labor  does  not  produce  the  adveise  personal  and  social 
outcomes  that  have  been  imputed  to  it,  it  remains  a  source  of  concern. 
"For  one  thing,"  Professor  Seeman  has  noted,  "work  life  absorbs  a  major 
portion  of  the  day,  and  an  ethical  stance  concerning  it  must  come  to 
terms  with  that  fact,  regardless  of  any  other  consequences  of  alienated 
labor.  For  another,  it  is  reasonable  to  argue  either  that  the  outcomes  we 
have  treated  constitute  an  insufficient  list  (the  data  say  little  about  the 
quality  of  family  life,  for  example)  or  that  the  consequences  will  reveal 
themselves  in  a  longer-term,  cumulative  way-for  example,  in  revolutions 
or  in  the  irregular  outbursts  of  a  wildcat  strike." 

Nor  can  it  be  assumed  that  men  alienated  from  their  daily  work  are 
indifferent  to  the  fact.  The  sort  of  data  required  to  examine  the  general- 
ization thesis  did  not  touch  on  such  matters.  It  cannot  be  said,  therefore, 
whether  or  not  the  worker  detests  his  work  and  is  angry  at  himself  for 

83 


doing  it.  All  that  the  data  do  allow  us  to  say,  Professor  Seenian  has 
pointed  out,  is  that  "people  can  work  out  fairly  effective  adjustments  to 
varied  kinds  of  work,  if  by  'effective'  we  simply  mean  leading  a  work  life 
that  has  little  generalized  effect  on  the  standard  forms  of  hating,  striving, 
withdrawing,  and  complaining  reviewed  here."  Thus,  although  the 
predictions  implicit  in  the  mass  society  generalization  thesis  may 
ultimately  prove  to  be  invalid,  the  ideological  indictment  of  industrial 
forms  that  breed  alienation  in  work  is  a  sepaiate  issue  and  one  still  most 
worthy  of  attention. 

The  concept  of  alienation  bears  on  some  of  the  most  urgent  problems 
confronting  our  society.  Yet  the  resounding  rhetoric  in  which  discussions 
of  alienation  are  often  couched  makes  it  too  easy  for  many  people  lo 
dismiss  the  ideas  along  with  the  words  as  baseless  pessimism.  Professor 
Seeman's  concern  as  a  scientist  has  been  to  help  turn  "the  parable  of 
alienation  into  a  proposition,"  or  rather  a  series  of  propositions  tlint  can 
be  accepted,  rejected,  or  amended  on  the  basis  of  deductions  drawn  from 
evidence  methodically  gathered  in  controlled  b'ut  real  situations,  As  a 
result  of  his  work,  we  begin  to  see  that  the  "pessimism"  of  the  mass 
society  radical  critique  is  not,  in  fact,  groundless. 

Much  more  research  on  problems  involving  alienation  needs  to  be 
carried  out  before  we  know  just  how  valid  the  critique  is  and,  more 
constructively,  how  much  insight  into  man's  social  behaviors  can  be 
gained  through  explorations  of  the  alienation  concept.  Professor  Seeman 
recently  outlined  a  general  program  of  research  to  be  done  in  this  area  and 
enumerated  several  studies  he  thought  would  be  useful.  In  that  paper, 
which  will  be  a  chapter  in  a  book  to  be  published  by  the  Russell  Sage 
Foundation,  the  investigator  also  described  the  unique  challenges  Hint 
such  research  makes  on  social  scientists. 

"The  fact  remains,"  Professor  Seeman  concluded,  "that  there  were  con- 
centration camps  and  genocide,  that  there  are  now  widely  disrespected 
qualities  in  American  life-including  a  widely  disrespected  war,  jit  the 
moment-and  that  there  will  be  further  urban  violence  born  of  frustrated 
hopes.  That  kind  of  past,  present,  and  future  create  a  special  tension  for 
the  sociologist  interested  in  the  problem  of  alienation.  It  is  the  tension 
between  keeping  a  craft  that  is  worthy  of  the  name,  and  at  the  same  time 
making  sociological  investigation  practically  and  morally  relevant.  The 
danger  on  the  craft  side  is  that  the  work  deteriorates  into  a  kind  of 
alienation  in  itself-bound  by  technical  rules,  limited  in  vision,  devoid  oi 
personal  involvement,  and  largely  oriented  to  careers.  The  danger  on  the 
side  of  relevance  is  that  this  deteriorates,  too-into  a  subtle  anli- 
intellectualism  that  is  impatient  with  any  thing  but  the  immediate;  or  into 
a  kind  of  self-indulgence  that  emphasizes  stance  over  analysis,  so  that 
what  becomes  crucial  is  one's  identification  (as  radical,  as  realist,  as 
humanist,  or  whatever).  My  hope  is  that  the  secularization  of  work  on 
alienation  can  continue  to  be  achieved  while  avoiding  both  these 
dangers-whieh  is  to  say  that  clarity  and  demonstration  can  be  success- 
fully wedded  to  scope  and  human  concern." 

84 


Research  Grant:  MH  10460 
Date  of  Interview.  October  1968 


References, 

Neal,  A.  G.;  and  Seeman,  S.  Organizations  and  powertessness:  A  test  of  the  mediation 

hypothesis.  American  Sociological  Review,  29  216-226,  1964. 
Seeman,    M.    On    the    meaning    of   alienation.    American    Sociological   Review, 
24:783-791,1959. 
Alienation  and  social  learning  in  a  reformatory.  American  Journal  of  Sociology, 

69:270-284,  1963. 
Alienation,  membership,  and  political  knowledge:  A  comparative  study.  Public 

Opinion  Quarterly,  30:353-367,  1966 
Status  and  identity:  The  problem  of  inauthenticity.  Pacific  Sociological  Review, 

9:67-73,  1966. 
Powerlessness  and  knowledge:  A  comparative  study  of  alienation  and  learning. 

Sodometry,  30:105-123;  1967. 
On  the  personal  consequences  of  alienation  in  work.  American  Sociological 

Review,  32:273-285,  1967. 

Alienation    and    engagement.    Chapter  for  a  forthcoming  publication  to  be 


published   under  the  auspices  of  the   Russell  Sage  Foundation  (edited   by  A. 
Campbell  and  P.  E.  Conveise). 
Seeman,  M,;  and  Evans,  J.  W.  Alienation  and  learning  in  a  hospital  setting.  American 

Sociological  Review,  27:772-782,  1962. 

Note. -In  addition  to  NIMH  grant  support,  the  investigator's  research  has  been 
aided  by  grants  from  the  University  of  California  and  The  Swedish  Social  Science 
Research  Council, 


85 


Investigator: 

Saul  Bernstein,  Ph.D. 
Boston  University 
Boston,  Mass. 

Prepared  by 

Clarissa  Wittenberg 

The  unruly  issues  of  riots,  slums,  and  racial  problems  are  now  of  major 
importance.  Our  state  of  information  is  extremely  limited,  and  major 
decisions  are  often  made  on  the  basis  of  scant  knowledge.  Saul  Bernstein, 
a  professor  of  social  group  work  at  Boston  University,  has  completed  two 
extensive  interview  studies  to  explore  these  and  related  issues.  His  focus 
has  been  on  our  most  alienated  young  people,  those  who  are  minority 
group  members  and  who  are  living  in  slums.  Initially  interested  in  delin- 
quency and  its  contemporary  forms,  he  then  turned  to  the  impact  of  the 
explosive  events  of  the  1 960's. 

Twice  in  the  mid-1 960's  Professor  Bernstein  traveled  to  nine  major 
American  cities  to  talk  to  people  working  closely  with  teenagers  in  the 
ghettos,  as  well  as  with  some  of  the  youngsters  themselves.  In  the  first 
study,  completed  in  1963, l  he  found  that  many  of  the  young  people  In 
this  country  live  in  terrible  housing  in  slums,  are  blocked  educationally, 
cannot  get  jobs,  are  undermined  by  family  problems,  and  are  caught  up  in 
destructive  cycles  with  new  babies  being  born  into  new  one-parcnl 
families.  Many  of  these  young  people  are  bitter  and  intensely  cynical,  and 
feel  hostile  and  destructive  towards  this  country.  Many  engage  in  delin- 
quent acts  and  are  in  trouble  with  the  police.  These  young  people  do  not 
feel  encouraged  by  the  new  legislative  landmarks  which  are  designed  Eo 
secure  their  rights.  If  anything,  many  felt  more  impatient  and  intolerant 
of  existing  inequities  than  ever  before.  Many  felt  that  this  country  had  let 
them  down.  Mr.  Bernstein  concluded  that  many  agencies  working  in  the 
slums  were  doing  good  work  but  against  great  obstacles,  and  he  especially 
singles  out  the  street  workers  as  of  great  importance  in  reaching  these 
alienated  young  people. 

In'the  mid-1960's  this  country  was  torn  by  riots  and  stunned  particular- 
ly by  the  Watts  riot.  Many  people  were  puzzled  and  shocked  because 
progress  had  been  made  in  civil  rights  legislation  and  the  antipoverty 
program  had  begun.  The  civil  rights  movement  at  that  time  was  also  strong 
and  tasting  the  fruits  of  success.  It  was  at  this  time  that  the  second  study 
was  planned  to  determine  if  any  of  the  socially  approved  forces  had 
touched  the  alienated  youth  with  the  same  force  as  had  the  riots,  An 


'Supported  by  grants  from  the  Duncan  Russell  Memorial  Delinquency  Committee 
of  the  United  Community  Services  of  Metropolitan  Boston  and  the  Permanent  Charity 
Fund  of  Boston. 

86 


exploration  of  the  role  of  these  young  people  in  the  riots  was  also 
planned. 

Dining  both  studies  the  investigator  visited  Boston,  Chicago,  Cleveland, 
Detioit,  Los  Angeles,  New  York,  Philadelphia,  and  Washington,  D.C.  The 
second  study  done  in  1965—66  substituted  Rochestci,  N.Y.,  for  San 
Francisco.  At  that  time  Rochester  had  had  riots  and  San  Francisco  had 
not.  The  design  in  both  studies  called  for  the  location  of  major  social 
agencies  working  with  young  people  in  the  slums,  and  to  inteiview  staff  in 
each  of  these  agencies.  In  addition  to  interviews  with  staff,  the  young 
people  themselves  were  intei  viewed  whenever  possible.  In  some  cases, 
staff  membcis  were  themselves  indigenous  workers  and  very  representative 
of  the  ghetto  population.  In  both  studies,  the  cities  were  selected  because 
they  had  large  numbeis  of  hostile,  alienated  young  people  and  because 
agencies  in  these  cities  had  long  expeiicnce  in  work  with  young  people. 
The  focus  was  on  the  poor,  including  members  of  minority  groups; 
Negroes,  Mexican-Americans,  Puerto  Ricans,  and  poor  whites. 

In  the  study  done  in  1965—66,  the  investigator  visited  nine  cities.  He 
held  inteiviews  with  289  people.  Most  of  those  interviewed  wcie  staff 
members  at  various  levels  of  responsibility  in  agencies  active  in  slum  areas. 
In  most  cities  there  weie  interviews  with  officials  of  the  Human  Relations 
Commission  or  its  local  equivalent.  Police,  particularly  those  dealing  with 
juveniles,  weie  included.  Educators  were  seen.  Staff  members  of  various 
poverty  piograms  and  of  community  planning  councils  were  included. 
Repiesentatives  of  such  Federal  agencies  as  the  Office  of  Juvenile  Delin- 
quency and  Youth  Development,  and  various  research  groups,  were  inter- 
viewed. Experimental  agencies,  such  as  Mobilization  for  Youth  and 
Haryou-Act  in  New  York,  and  the  Cleveland  Community  Action  for 
Youth,  were  included,  as  were  new  training  programs  such  as  Manpower  in 
Rochester.  Limited  time  of  the  second  study  and  the  inaccessibility  of  the 
young  people  themselves  prevented  more  than  a  small  number  of  inter- 
views with  really  alienated  young  people  themselves.  A  number  of 
representatives  of  militant  groups,  such  as  Saul  Alinsky's  Industrial  Areas 
Foundation,  were  interviewed.  AH  interviews  took  place  between 
December  1 965  and  June  1 966.  In  many  cases  group  interviews  were  held. 

This  technique  resulted  in  a  broad  and  detailed  series  of  observations  of 
experts  and  young  people  about  some  of  the  major  events  of  the 
mid-1 960's,  While  events  have  been  occurring  at  rapid  speed  in  this  area, 
this  study  provides  a  valuable  cross  section  of  opinion  at  that  time.  The 
investigator  acknowledges  that  the  research  is  limited  by  several  factors. 
Obviously,  not  all  alienated  youth  come  into  contact  with  such  agencies 
and  the  experts  interviewed  may  have  some  biases.  The  study  is  treated  as 
documentary  rather  than  as  "hard"  research. 

The  major  question  of  the  study  was  to  find  if  we  as  a  society  had 
found  any  way  other  than  riots  to  touch  the  alienated  young  person  and 
help  him  change  his  status,  There  is  also  great  concern  about  the  role  of 
the  ghetto  youth  in  these  massive  riots.  Although  many  things  have  oc- 
curred to  correct  injustices  and  open  up  opportunities,  it  is  still  question- 
able if  they  have  made  sufficient  impact.  The  study  was  designed  to 

87 


increase  our  information  about  how  these  young  people  feel  about  ric 
about  legislative  landmarks,  protest  marches,  desegregated  schoc 
"black"  schools,  etc. 

The  first  study  (in  1963)  showed  that  there  are  many  people  caught 
our  slums  in  very  destructive  patterns.  Some  changes  in  patterns  of  del 
quent  gangs  have  occurred.  The  highly  structured  large  gangs  appear 
have   diminished,  although   not   to    have  disappeared.   Smaller  grou 
referred  to  as  clusters  tend  to  piedominate  now.  Although  it  may 
reassuring  to  see  the  disappearance  of  the  highly  visible  signs  of  gitii] 
such  as  matching  leather  jackets  or  gang  giaffiti  on  walls,  this  may  be  cl 
to  the  accumulated  pressure  brought  to  bear  on  visible  gangs,  rather  (h 
to   their  lack  of  strength.  The  gangs  may   have  "learned  the  game 
Although    rumbles   on    a    large   scale   have   decreased,   "snagging" 
"Japping"  was  commonly  mentioned.  In  this  form  of  fighting,  a  looko 
watches  the  regular  movements  of  one  or  two  members  of  an  antagonist 
gang.  When  it  is  established  that  a  suitable  place  will  be  passed  by  t! 
victim  at  a  predictable  time,  about  six  of  the  aggressors  go  homo,  was 
shave,  put  on  good  suits,  and  then  saunter  towards  that  spot.  They  a 
careful  to  go  in  pairs  and  not  to  show  in  any  way  by  their  behavior  what 
planned.  When  the  victim  comes  along,  they  give  him  a  bad  beating  an 
then  go  casually  home  to  change  clothes.  In  some  places  the  ritual  \v« 
different  so  as  not  to  risk  their  good  clothes.  Clothing  is  very  impoiUnl  i 
the  gang  psychology.  Another  form  of  gang  fighting  mentioned  is  the  "fa 
one."  Heie  a  repiesentative  of  each  group  fights  without  weapons  t 
assistance.  This,  however,  calls  for  a  high  level  of  discipline  which  is  rare! 
achieved.  This  is  relatively  infrequent  as  the  fighters  are  in  a  very  Icnst 
public,  and  vulneiable  position  as  well. 

Most  of  the  aggressive  incidents  occur  between  groups  with  the  sail) 
ethnic  or  racial  background,  although  in  some  places  even  the  nios 
confined  ghetto  population  is  quite  mobile  and  there  are  some  incident 
where  groups  have  traveled  to  other  areas  for  conflict. 

The  study  revealed  that  although  these  bitter,  hostile  young  pcoplt 
would  seem  ripe  recruits  for  militant  01  protest  groups  that  relatively  few 
had  joined.  The  Black  Muslims,  for  example,  had  little  to  offer  in  terms  oJ 
meeting  concrete  needs,  such  as  jobs.  The  NAACP  methods  often  seen 
too  slow  and  removed  to  be  attractive  to  these  young  people.  However, 
some  young  people,  generally  the  less  hostile  ones,  have  joined  NAACP.  Id 
Boston  it  was  reported  that  a  small  number  of  white  gang  members  were 
recruited  by  the  American  Nazi  Party. 

The  first  study  also  examined  the  role  of  the  "street  worker."  Agencies 
in  slum  areas  have  found  that  "detached"  workers  can  move  into  the 
environment  of  the  young  people  and  reach  them  in  a  way  no  office- 
bound  worker  could  do.  They  provide  a  link  between  agencies  and  some 
young  people  who  have  lost  contact  with  all  legitimate  agencies  or  social 
groups.  Street  workers  vary;  some  have  master's  degrees,  some  have  not 
yet  finished  high  school.  Most  have  undergraduate  degrees.  Some  have 
delinquent  or  prison  backgrounds  and  some  are  from  very  stable  middle- 
class  backgrounds.  As  a  group  they  tend  to  be  very  active  and  nol 


attracted  by  a  9  to  5  life.  Most  are  deeply  involved  in  helping  people  find 
themselves  and  achieve  some  success.  They  tend  to  be  not  well  accepted 
into  any  one  professional  group.  They  are  "lonely"  professionals. 
Agencies  have  a  hard  time  retaining  street  workers,  and  the  demands  of 
the  job  are  very  grueling,  so  most  workers  tend  to  be  in  their  twenties  or 
thirties.  Job  opportunity  tends  to  be  limited  in  terms  of  opportunity  for 
advancement  and  increased  income.  These  issues  are  important  as  more 
and  more  innovative  agencies  designed  to  serve  the  ghetto  population 
depend  heavily  upon  the  services  of  the  street  worker.  Social  work  is 
considered  the  nearest  profession.  Mr.  Bernstein  comments  that  street 
work,  which  has  been  estimated  to  cost  about  $200  to  $600  annually  foi 
each  youngster,  is  little  enough  to  pay  for  work  which  genuinely  reaches 
them  and  often  their  families. 

After  examining   patterns  and   services  and  the  state  of  some  veiy 

alienated  young  people  in  the  first  study,  the  investigator  turned  to  what 
was  happening  and  what  alternatives  theie  might  be  to  the  violence  oc- 
cuning  in  our  cities.  An  early  task  was  the  division  of  the  various  riots  and 
incidents  into  broad  categories.  In  the  mid-1 960's  riots  occurred  that  were 
the  result  of  spontaneous  events,  e.g.,  the  riot  in  Boston  following  a  dance 
which  excluded  large  numbers  of  young  people  who  could  not  be  ac- 
commodated. Other  riots  were  between  racial  or  ethnic  groups,  such  as 
the  incident  sometimes  called  Watts  II  on  March  15,  1966,  between 
Mexican-Americans  and  Negroes.  However,  violence  within  the  ghetto 
itself  was  the  most  serious  and  disturbing  type.  At  that  time  Negroes  were 
primarily  involved,  although  some  episodes  were  thought  to  have  had  their 
beginnings  in  the  tensions  of  Puerto  Ricans  or  Mexican-Americans. 
Despite  much  "get  whitey"  talk  and  the  destruction  of  white-owned 
businesses,  the  aggression  at  the  time  of  the  study  was  largely  confined  to 
slum  areas.  White  men  were  injured  primarily  as  they  came  into  the  riot 
area  in  an  official  capacity,  such  as  happened  with  police  or  firemen. 
Threats  to  burn  and  loot  white  areas  did  not  materialize. 

To  realize  the  magnitude  of  the  riots  of  the  mid-1 960's  a  review  of  the 
McCone  Commission  figures  is  valuable.  Although  Watts  was  the  most 
memorable  riot  of  that  period,  many  others  occurred.  Many,  many  people 


McCone  Commission  Report  summary  of  riots  occurring  in  1964: 


City 

Date 

Killed 

Injured 

Arrests 

Stores 
Damaged 

New  York  

July  1  8-23  

1 

144 

519 

541 

July  24-25  

4 

350 

976 

204 

Jersey  City  

Aug.  2-4  
Aug.  11-13  

0 
0 

46 
8 

52 
65 

71 
20 

Aug.  11-13  

o 

6 

18 

17 

Aug.  16-17 

0 

57 

80 

2 

Dixmoor  (Phila.)  .  , 

Aug.  28-30  

0 

341 

774 

225 

89 


were  injured,  many  arrested,  and  some  killed.  To  make  the  figures  imm 
dramatic,  it  must  be  remembered  that  only  a  small  fraction  of  Iliok 
involved  were  arrested  or  noted  in  official  statistics.  It  is  speculated,  too 
that  many  more  who  never  took  part  still  tacitly  supported  the  rioting. 

All  of  these  occurred  between  July  18  and  August  30,  a  6-weck  purm? 
Five  dead,  a  total  of  952  injured,  2,484  arested,  and  1,080  slorr- 
damaged.  Despite  all  this,  the  riot  in  Watts  overshadowed  the  icst.  Ir 
began  on  August  11,  1965,  a  Wednesday,  and  ended  the  following 
Tuesday,  August  17.  It  was  estimated  that  at  times  as  many  as  tcr 
thousand  Negroes  participated.  This,  however,  was  still  only  2  percent  of 
the  population  living  in  the  riot  aiea.  The  size  of  the  riot  area  was  ar. 
incredible  46.5  squaie  miles.  The  dead  totaled  34,  the  injured  1,032 
arrests  3,952,  and  damage  was  estimated  at  about  $40  million.  Only  ois 
public  building  was  destroyed  and  only  14  damaged  or  burned.  Tlwiun 
interesting  issue,  as  these  buildings,  while  not  attractive  to  looters,  dv 
symbolize  the  dominant  "establishment."  Although  there  is  always 
present  the  explanation  of  riots  being  a  spontaneous  mass  protest  wlicw 
people  get  out  of  control  while  attempting  to  express  a  protest  agaimi 
degrading  living  conditions,  this  explanation  fails  to  account  for  (lie  con- 
trol exhibited  in  some  areas  and  the  relative  immunity  given,  for  instance, 
to  schools,  normally  a  target  for  vandals.  Rioters  in  Watts  were  Jilso  ob- 
served stopping  at  traffic  lights  and  driving  with  caution.  Examples  were 
given  during  the  study  interviews  of  looters  apologizing  when  bumping 
into  each  other.  There  was  considerable  evidence  as  well  that  riots  nmylio 
provoked  or  related  to  specific  incidents  or  situations  rather  than  emerg- 
ing spontaneously  from  the  blue. 

Some  situations  were  cited  as  definite  causes  of  riots.  For  example, 
Garfield  Park  in  Chicago,  a  predominantly  Negro  area,  had  a  fire  station 
with  an  all-white  staff  until  after  the  riot  when  it  was  integrated.  The 
California  vote  defeating  Proposition  14,  an  open-housing  ordinance,  was 
considered  an  initant  in  Watts.  The  inadequate  public  transportation  in 
Los  Angeles  was  also  cited.  A  man  looking  for  work  in  Los  Angeles  who 
lived  in  Watts  might  have  to  spend  several  hotus  on  a  bus  and  pay  it  lound 
trip  cost  of  almost  a  dollar.  Budd  Schulberg,  who  later  established  a 
Writer's  Workshop  in  Watts,  told  of  seeing  a  group  on  a  street  in  Walts.  .A 
6-month-old  baby  had  died.  The  mother's  grief  was  intensified  by  (lie 
bitter  knowledge  that  the  prompt  arrival  of  an  ambulance  and  a  hospitJf 
closer  than  the  County  General  might  have  saved  her  child.  In  April  of 
1 966  there  was  still  no  public  hospital  in  Watts,  and  Los  Angeles  voleis 
later  rejected  a  bond  issue  to  construct  one. 

Others  interviewed  told  of  comments  by  law  enforcement  officers  that 
were  irritants,  Particularly  cited  were  "abrasive"  remarks  by  Los  Angeles 
Police  Commissioner  Parker.  A  comment  he  had  made  in  1958,  that 
Negroes  committed  1 1  times  the  major  crimes  as  other  races,  was  siiii 
remembered  with  bitterness  by  Negroes  in  that  area. 

The  rigidity  of  the  Boston  School  Committee  about  de  facto  segrega- 
tion was  considered  an  outrage.  Even  the  withholding  of  State  funds  for 
education,  because  of  the  unwillingness  of  the  Boston  School  Committee 

90 


to  prepare  and  put  into  effect  adequate  measuies  for  the  desegregation  of 
public  schools,  did  not  produce  significant  change  in  1966.  Elections  for 
posts  on  the  School  Committee,  which  produced  large  votes  for  the  most 
intransigent  members  and  defeat  for  the  ones  in  favor  of  desegregation, 
added  to  the  affront  to  the  Negroes. 

Heat,  "the  long  hot  summer,"  adds  to  the  tensions  and  problems  of 
crowded  living  and  may  bring  a  state  of  irritability  that  is  explosive. 

Accidents  can  trigger  incidents.  A  fire  truck  on  an  emeigency  call  hit  a 
Negro  woman  and  killed  her.  Since  the  fire  station  involved  was  all-white 
and  in  a  Negro  area  and  already  the  source  of  tension,  the  rumor  spread 
that  the  killing  was  on  purpose. 

The  presence  or  absence  of  social  controls  is  an  important  element.  In 
Philadelphia,  at  the  "Wall"  of  the  Girard  College,  a  crisis  was  described 
which  was  headed  off  on  at  least  one  occasion  by  the  strong  activity  of 
the  Human  Relations  Commission  which  marshalled  clergymen,  probation 
officeis,  police,  street  workers,  and  others  to  help  keep  the  gang  members 
who  were  gathering  under  control,  Other  types  of  social  control  are 
punitive  and  may  work  temporarily  but  appear  to  be  self-defeating. 

For  instance  the  mayor  of  a  riot-torn  area  in  Illinois  said  he  would  meet 
with  Negro  ministers  but  not  with  "violators  or  demonstrators."  Participa- 
tion in  riots  was  justification  for  being  sent  eviction  notices  if  participants 
Jived  in  public  housing.  In  other  places  parents  in  public  housing  were  very 
concerned  about  involvement  of  their  children  in  any  kind  of  a  protest  for 
fear  it  would  lead  to  the  family  being  evicted.  Officials  of  some  cities  had 
considered  the  legality  of  cutting  people  off  public  welfare  or  un- 
employment compensation.  The  assumption  is  that  those  who  are  arrested 
are  guilty  and  were  more  active  in  rioting  than  those  who  were  not 
arrested,  which  may  not  necessarily  be  true.  These  measures,  such  as 
eviction,  are  punishment  beyond  that  established  for  criminal  acts  and 
make  life  worse  for  those  affected.  The  accessibility  of  public  officials  to 
those  in  the  ghettos  is  very  important  and  can  reduce  the  impulse  to  riot. 


Role  of  the  Alienated  Young  Person  in  the  Riots 

Those  interviewed  in  this  study  described  considerable  activity  by  the 
young  people  in  the  ghettos.  They  looted,  burned,  and  fought  the  police. 
Some  were  members  of  gangs,  although  a  more  frequent  pattern  was  for 
small  cliques  to  riot  together.  These  were  units  of  three  to  five  youngsters. 
The  older  teenagers  rather  than  the  younger,  and  boys  rather  than  girls, 
predominated.  It  was  the  general  finding  that  these  young  people  </W  not 
plan  the  riots.  Many  of  the  young  people  who  took  part  were  in  their 
twenties  or  thirties.  Strong  feelings  were  expressed  by  ghetto  youth 
against  the  middle-class  Negroes  who  attempted  to  stop  the  riots.  One 
group  of  teenagers  in  Rochester  articulated  this  antagonism,  claiming  that 
middle-class  Negroes  thought  that  the  white  reaction  to  the  riot  would 
hurt  them.  These  same  young  people  were  bitter  that  many  better-off 
Negros  moved  out  of  the  bad  areas  and  then  abandoned  all  responsibility 

91 


for  them.  The  investigator  notes  that,  despite  the  concern  for  the  ghc 
problems  shown  by  middle-class  Negroes  interviewed  in  this  study,  then 
great  resentment  towards  them  by  those  still  in  the  ghetto. 

A  wide  range  of  feelings  was  expressed  about  the  riots.  Some  you 
people  admitted  that  they  enjoyed  them.  Others  said  they  were  dangero 
and  foolish.  Some  felt  that  they  are  essential  to  attract  attention  to  t 
ghettos.  One  comment  was,  "It  is  better  to  spill  blood  in  Watts  than 
Viet  Nam."  Theie  is  little  doubt  that  the  liotsgive  people  in  the  ghetto 
sense  of  community  and  power.  In  Los  Angeles  it  was  said  that  particlp 
tion  in  the  riots  became  a  status  symbol,  and  many  felt  good  to  have  bet 
a  part  of  it.  Many  spoke  of  the  dangers  of  being  shot,  hurt,  or  arrcstd 
Young  people  in  Boston  spoke  of  envying  Watts  and  yet  did  not  speak < 
riots  in  their  own  area.  Detroit  and  Washington  seemed  calmer  than  othi 
cities,  yet  both  had  riots  later.  At  the  time  of  the  study,  the  riots  scemc 
very  remote  and  unrelated  to  the  Pueito  Ricans  and  Mexican-American 
interviewed. 

There  is  tremendous  complexity  in  the  reactions  to  riots.  The  impact  o 
the  experience  itself  and  its  realities,  such  as  food  shortage,  jail,  etc. 
bring  about  additional  factors  that  become  added  to  those  present  prior  U 
the  riots.  The  mass  news  media  and  many  civic  leaders  add  interpretation1 
and  give  reasons,  and  these  all  become  incorporated  into  the  discussion 
The  reasons  given  after  a  riot  may  be  quite  different  from  the  motivations 
of  the  riot  period  itself.  Still,  an  overwhelming  certainty  was  that  for  tltc 
people  in  this  study  the  reality  of  the  unbearable  living  conditions  and  (lie 
profound  feeling  of  helplessness  was  a  major  factor  in  every  riot. 


Agency  Activity  During  Riots 

Street  workers  who  had  established  influential  relationships  with  young 
people  on  the  streets  were  able  to  make  major  contributions.  In  Gar  field 
Park,.  Chicago,  the  workers  were  able  to  persuade  the  young  people  they 
knew  to  leave  the  riot  area.  In  other  cases  the  street  workeis  were  able  to 
enlist  gang  leaders  in  the  effort  to  keep  their  friends  out  of  the  riot 
activity.  Several  agencies  took  young  people  away  from  the  areas  to  lessen 
the  opportunity  for  involvement,  During  the  Watts  riot  the  staff  of  the 
Special  Service  for  Groups  took  food  to  desperate  areas.  Later,  when  it 
was  considered  unsafe  for  whites  to  enter  the  area,  the  Negro  staff  carried 
on. 

All  of  the  cities  need  more  street  workers  in  the  rough  areas.  The  street 
worker  is  seen  by  the  militant  Negro  as  a  threat  because  of  his  commit- 
ment to  nonviolence.  Mr.  Bernstein  quotes  a  letter  sent  in  plain  envelope 
to  three  street  workers  at  Lawndale  Neighborhood  Services  of  Chicago 
Youth  Centers: 

"Grettings  Brother: 

"Have  you  ever  considered  WHY  the  GREAT  WHITE  FATHER  is 
continuing  to  spend  MORE  money  on  the  various  street  work  pro 

92 


giams  throughout  the  entire  country  and  especially  in  the  BLACK 
GHETTOS.  Also  why  the  GREAT  WHITE  FATHER  is  expanding 
these  piograms  to  include  community  organizations  If  you  are  aware 
of  the  current  struggle  being  waged  by  Black  Americans  to  be  recog- 
nized as  human  beings  in  the  so-called  'land  of  the  free  and  home  of 
the  brave,'  it  is  not  difficult  to  arrive  at  the  most  logical  answer  as  to 
why  WHITEY  is  expanding  the  program. 

"The  white  man  wants  to  contain  our  people,  to  keep  them  from 
rebelling  against  living  in  slum  housing,  receiving  poor  education,  and 
thus  the  worst  jobs,  if  any  at  all.  The  people  who  run  America  do  not 
want  the  Afro-American  to  know  who  he  is,  to  have  an  identity,  to 
know  of  his  glorious  past,  and  the  prospects  of  an  even  richer  futiue 
if  they  can  get  Charlie  off  their  backs. 

"Today  there  is  a  fierce  battle  being  waged  for  the  minds  of  the 
youth  of  America,  especially  Black  Youth.  For  young  people  are 
invariably  more  idealistic  than  older  people,  and  thus  are  not  likely 
to  go  for  as  much  bull-as  older  people.  Thus,  it  is  principally  the 
Black  Youth  who  fought  the  police  during  the  ziots  of  last  summer, 
age  range  from  14  to  19  years  old. 

"Because  Mister  Charlie  pays  your  salary  the  same  as  he  pays  me, 
ask  yourself,  if  the  real-breaks  out  here,  what  will  be  my  ROLE?  If 
your  wife  and  children  are  in  the  streets  during  a  riot,  the  BIG 
WHITE  COP  will  crack  their  heads  just  like  any  Negro's  head.  MY 
BROTHER,  what  will  you  do  with  your  group?  Will  it  be  a,  'cool  it, 
baby,  they  ain't  done  no  thin'  to  us,1  or  will  it  be  a  'defend  your- 
selves, brothers'?  Things  are  bound  to  get  worse  all  over.  The  War  on 
Poverty  is  a  farce,  a  throwing  of  ciumbs  to  the  poor,  both  black  and 
white,  to  stem  the  tide  of  rebellion. 

"Every  Afro-American  street  worker  has  a  very  important  role  to 
play  in  the  coming  days  ahead.  In  the  language  of  the  street,  you  can 
teach  the  young  brothers  and  sisters  to  be  proud  to  be  BLACK.  You 
can  help  destroy  the  often  subconscious  inferiority  complex  that 
exists  in  many  of  our  ghetto  youth.  BROTHER,  it  is  a  question  of 
whether  or  not  you  will  truly  be  a  BLACK  MAN  who  cares  above  all 
else  about  the  hopes  and  aspirations  of  his  people.  The  choice  is 
yours  to  make  and  I  have  confidence  that  you  will  not  be  a  SELL- 
OUT. The  fight  is  ours  and  we  cannot  afford  to  not  be  victorious. 

Your  Soul  Brother" 


In  addition  to  the  efforts  of  the  street  workers,  some  agencies  were  able 
to  make  and  keep  active  telephone  contact  with  clients  in  the  affected 
areas.  This  is  an  especially  good  technique  for  verifying  or  defusing 
rumors. 

93 


A  common  feeling  among  Negioes,  even  those  who  would  work  to 
prevent  a  riot,  is  that  no  Negro  can  totally  regret  that  riots  have  occurred- 
Although  many  people  suffer  and  there  is  a  trauma  involved,  there  are 
definite  gains  in  terms  of  community  awakening.  There  is  heightened 
sensitivity  on  the  part  of  police  and  politicians  to  their  handling.  Whites 
have  a  range  of  reactions  from  dedication,  to  eradicating  the  basic  wrong*, 
to  buying  guns.  Riots  in  some  situations  increased  communication  and  in 
others  intensified  the  polarization. 


Participation  in  the  Civil  Rights  Movement 

The  civil  rights  movement  in  this  context  is  described  as  a  broad 
combination  of  legislation,  oiganizations,  public  opinion,  and  activities 
aimed  to  extend  all  civil  rights  to  those  who  have  been  deprived  of  them. 
The  means  are  many,  including  marches,  demonstrations,  sit-ins,  boycotts, 
political  pressure,  etc.  In  the  early  1960's  many  white  people  were 
involved  as  well  as  members  of  minority  groups.  At  the  time  of  the  study 
some  militant  groups  of  Negroes  had  given  up  on  the  civil  rights  move- 
ment as  an  effective  force.  However,  they  must  also  be  included  here 
because  of  their  actual  and  potential  impact  on  alienated  youth.  The  civil 
rights  movement  has  been  concerned  with  human  rights  as  well  as  strictly 
civil  rights  and  often  includes  a  strong  appeal  to  decency  as  well  as  to  law 
enforcement. 

Little  participation  in  civil  rights  activity  on  the  part  of  alienated  youth 
was  discoveied  by  this  study.  One  group  of  boys  in  Chicago  had  helped  to 
promote  a  large  meeting  to  collect  food  and  money  to  be  sent  to  Negroes 
who  had  been  cut  off  from  welfare.  This  had  occurred  when  they  had 
registered  to  vote  in  a  Southern  community.  Some  other  participation 
took  place  largely  through  agencies.  For  instance,  Mobilization  for  Youth 
in  New  York  took  a  large  number  of  ghetto  residents  to  Washington  for 
the  March  on  Washington  in  1963.  By  and  large,  participation  was  limited 
and  almost  accidental.  If  a  protest  march  went  through  their  neighbor- 
hood, they  might  join  in.  The  young  people  in  the  civil  rights  movement 
have  been  those  whose  motivations  and  aspirations  have  been  more  clearly 
defined  than  those  of  the  young  people  here  under  study.  By  and  large, 
the  young  people  who  are  most  alienated  are  also  bitter  and  less  likely  to 
participate  in  a  civil  rights  movement.  They  are  quicker  to  strike  out, 
more  hostile  to  the  police,  and  less  disciplined.  Some  groups  would  even 
discourage  their  participation  so  as  not  to  jeopardize  their  cause  or 
inflame  a  sensitive  situation.  The  more  quick-tempered  prefer  to  work  at 
things  in  a  more  direct  way.  The  staff  at  Haryou-Act  in  Harlem  described 
a  group  that  was  concerned  about  the  lack  of  toilets  at  a  playground, 
They  then  got  publicity  for  the  problem  by  urinating  in  cups  at  the 
playground.  They  did  get  the  toilets.  Many  of  the  most  alienated  young 
people  did  not  even  really  understand  the  civil  rights  movement.  Many  in 

94 


the  North  thought  it  was  only  fo:  'colored  people  in  the  South."  The 
delayed  results  of  the  civil  rights  movement  are  not  attiactive  to  them. 
This  group  has  had  little  experience  with  altruism  and  cannot  believe  that 
anyone  does  anything  for  a  reason  other  than  his  own  gain. 

It  was  clear  from  the  interviews  that  the  nonviolent  theme  is  not 
compatible  with  the  impulses  of  the  angry  young  men  in  the  ghetto.  They 
feel  a  strong  need  to  retaliate.  Although  many  situations  have  been 
corrected  legally,  the  problems  still  exist  in  an  extremely  frustrating  form. 
For  instance,  these  young  men  know  that  legally  many  restaurants  are 
open  to  them  and  that  stores  cannot  refuse  to  serve  them.  They  also  know 
that  they  will  be  ill-received  or  self-conscious  if  they  venture  out  of  home 
territory.  Their  clothing  or  their  hairdos  will  mark  them  as  different.  They 
see  the  stores  and  do  not  have  the  money  to  patronize  them.  The  jobs  that 
are  available  to  them  pay  so  little  that  often  they  do  not  materially 
advance  their  ability  to  have  the  things  they  so  desire.  They  see  the  quick 
financial  rewards  that  come  from  illicit  operations.  They  resent  having  to 
"break  their  backs"  to  earn  a  legitimate  salary  and  yet  know  that  "their 
backs"  are  all  they  have  to  offer  due  to  their  poor  educations. 

This  study  showed  that  the  civil  rights  movement  has  to  a  certain 
extent  increased  the  awareness  of  the  average  minority  group  member  of 
the  discrimination  that  he  suffers.  The  need  for  an  increase  in  black  pride 
and  a  self-respecting  racial  identity  is  crucial.  There  is  still  the  question  as 
to  whether  any  attempt  to  revive  dignity  can  be  totally  successful  without 
significant  changes  being  made  in  the  slums  and  in  employment,  and  in 
general  acceptance  and  respect  on  the  part  of  the  dominant  community. 

At  the  time  of  the  study,  the  heroes  of  the  civil  lights  movement  were 
little  known  to  ghetto  youth.  The  Reverend  Martin  Luther  King,  Jr., 
known  to  everyone  since  his  death,  was  relatively  little  known  by  ghetto 
youth  in  most  cities,  although  he  had  been  widely  honored  in  this  country 
and  abroad.  Malcolm  X  was  an  authentic  object  of  hero  worship.  During 
the  riot  in  Harlem  in  1964,  youth  were  quoted  as  chanting:  "We  want 
Malcolm  X."  Many  knew  Cassius  Clay  whom  they  admired  for  his  boxing 
skill.  Feelings  were  mixed  regarding  his  lack  of  humility.  In  Roxbury,  the 
Boston  Celtics  were  highly  regarded  by  the  Negro  youngsters. 

The  special  emphasis  on  the  concept  of  "black"  or  "Afro-American," 
with  its  repudiation  of  slavery  and  its  labels,  is  congenial  to  the  most 
alienated  persons  in  the  ghettos.  The  concept  of  Black  superiority  is  wel- 
come to  them.  However,  this  study  showed  that,  despite  the  compatibility 
of  the  concept,  few  of  the  young  people  joined  the  movements;  most 
remained  as  isolated  as  before  from  the  larger  society. 

Puerto  Ricans  and  Mexican-Americans  have  been  much  less  involved 
than  Negroes  in  the  civil  rights  activities.  The  reality  of  Puerto  Rico  as  a 
place  and  the  recognizable  and  respected  culture  helps  them  tolerate  the 
problems  here.  This  is  true  of  Mexicans  as  well.  The  emergence  of  the  new 
Black  African  countries  is  important  but  very  remote  to  most  American 

95 


Negroes.  While  the  Puerto  Rican  can  and  does  return  to  Puerto  Rico,  the 
average  American  Negro  does  not  go  to  Africa,  and,  if  he  does,  he  may 
feel  more  alienated  and  foreign  than  in  the  United  States.  Although  the 
militants  have  revived  or  made  ties  to  Africa,  by  and  large  the  American 
Negro  feels  that,  good  or  bad,  this  is  his  country. 


Antipoverty  Programs 

The  War  on  Poverty  generated  a  great  many  programs  to  attempt  to 
attack  serious  problems.  Of  those  specifically  designed  to  help  adoles- 
cents, perhaps  Job  Corps  and  Neighborhood  Youth  Corps  arc  most 
pertinent. 

Job  Corps  Camps  were  set  up  away  from  slum  areas.  They  offeied 
healthy  environments,  good  food,  remedial  education,  and  job  training. 
This  might  have  been  beneficial  for  these  particularly  unequipped  and 
hostile  young  people,  except  that  a  few  things  were  wrong.  The  publicity 
was  attractive,  but  the  delays  in  processing  applications  caused  people  to 
become  discouraged.  The  location  of  the  camps  away  from  home  sur- 
roundings made  some  uneasy.  The  similarity  for  some  youth  of  being  sent 
away  to  training  school  was  too  marked.  Agencies  which  did  send  young 
men  had  a  hard  time  finding  out  what  happened  to  them.  Information  was 
rarely  sent  to  the  referring  agency  when  a  young  man  dropped  out  or  was 
asked  to  leave.  So,  for  a  variety  of  reasons,  the  Job  Corps  did  not  make  a 
noticeable  mark  on  alienated  youth  in  the  cities  studied. 

The  Neighborhood  Youth  Corps  included  programs  of  jobs  for  school 
dropouts,  special  intensive  summer  job  programs,  and  activities  for  in- 
school  youth.  Social  agencies,  government  offices,  and  industry  par- 
ticipated as  work  stations.  Youth  whose  families  had  incomes  al  the 
poverty  level  were  acceptable.  Usually  assignments  were  for  about  20  to 
24  hours  a  week  for  those  who  were  out  of  school  and  less  for  (hose  in 
school.  The  pay  rate  was  about  $1 .25.  A  common  complaint  was  thai  (his 
rate  was  too  low.  Many  participants  regarded  the  assignment  as  work  and 
not  training  and  felt  that  it  was  not  enough  money.  Delays  in  issuing 
checks  and  other  such  problems  lowered  motivation  to  stay  on  in  some 
cases.  Even  the  "poverty"  label  was  offensive  to  some.  After  6  months 
those  out  of  school  had  to  resume  some  type  of  educational  program  or 
be  dropped. 

The  Neighborhood  Youth  Corps  was  most  successful  where  the  policy 
was  clear  and  counseling  was  an  integral  part  of  the  program.  The  naUurc 
of  the  work  assigned  and  its  potential  benefit  to  the  young  person  were 
also  important.  Most  members  valued  the  opportunity  to  use  business 
machines  and  other  skilled  types  of  jobs  much  more  than  the  menial  jobs 
usually  available  to  them. 

Agencies  which  employed  these  young  people  found  that  their  early 
employment  phase  was  often  difficult.  A  combination  of  patience,  under- 
standing, and  firmness  was  needed  to  deal  with  absences,  unexplained 
lateness,  or  early  leaving.  Some  young  people  were  in  agencies  whose  staff 

96 


understood  the  hair  styles  or  dress  of  the  youth  and  the  identity  issues 
involved,  but  also  felt  these  posed  a  barrier  to  later  employment. 
Language  and  attitudes  about  authority  also  caused  problems.  The  way 
that  each  issue  was  handled  was  important  in  each  case.  The  youth  had  to 
be  accepted  as  they  were  at  the  beginning,  and  later  the  realities  of  the 
employment  market  were  introduced. 

Local  programs  were  developed  in  various  cities  with  the  approval  of 
the  local  antipoverty  organization.  Mr.  Bernstein  reports  on  a  few  that 
seemed  successful  or  interesting. 

C/wa£o-STREETS  (Socialization,  Training,  Education,  and  Employment 
Technical  Services) 

This  was  a  cooperative  venture  by  the  Chicago  Boys'  Clubs,  the  Chicago 
Youth  Centers,  Chicago  Federation  of  Settlements  and  Neighborhood 
Centers,  and  the  YMCA.  The  purposes  in  general  were  to  enable  dis- 
advantaged  youth  to  function  better  in  relation  to  requiiements  of  our 
society.  Other  goals  were  to  make  fuller  use  of  social  welfare  and  other 
systems,  to  help  the  youth  move  toward  adequate  education,  satisfying 
jobs,  healthy  maniages,  and  so  on.  The  neighborhoods  selected  for  service 
were  among  those  designated  by  the  Chicago  Committee  on  Urban  Oppor- 
tunity as  having  the  greatest  poverty  and  related  problems. 

The  main  feature  of  STREETS  was  the  hiring  of  neighborhood  adults 
and  youth  to  help  in  the  work  of  the  agency.  The  youth  worked  primarily 
with  younger  children.  Those  with  leadership  quality  were  sought.  The 
adults,  called  neighborhood  aides,  shared,  with  the  professional  staff,  in 
the  supervision  of  the  youth,  called  program  aides.  The  aides  also 
benefited  from  the  agencies'  programs  as  well  as  promoting  them  for 
others.  The  program  was  set  up  in  units  designed  to  serve  1 ,500  youths. 
These  were  eight  units  and  a  staff  of  488  to  serve  12,000.  Those  inter- 
viewed were  enthusiastic  about  the  program  although  some  aides  did  not 
achieve  the  level  originally  set  for  them.  The  respondents  felt  that  great 
success  had  been  achieved  in  some  areas.  They  felt  that  some  aides  learned 
to  give  up  antisocial  acts,  assumed  great  responsibility  for  children,  and 
returned  to  school.  They  also  carried  over  to  their  own  peer  group  what 
they  had  learned. 

Central  to  this  success  was  the  familiarity  of  the  agencies  involved  with 
the  neighborhoods  and  clientele.  They  already  had  skill  in  working  with 
alienated  youth  and  this  made  their  commitment  to  the  most  difficult  of 
the  young  people  moie  durable.  They  seem  to  have  a  good  sense  of  the 
types  of  jobs  aides  can  perform,  and,  in  general,  the  program  incorporates 
the  advantages  of  using  indigenous  workers.  Aides  received  salaries  of 
about  $45  for  a  30-hour  week. 

Detroit— SWEEP  (Summer  Weekend  Evening  Emergency  Program) 

This  is  a  summer  weekend  program  conducted  by  the  Neighborhood 
Service  Organization  in  high  delinquency  and  poverty  areas,  in  coopera- 
tion with  the  Youth  Bureau  of  the  Police  Department,  with  schools, 
universities,  museums,  and  others.  It  was  for  girls  and  boys,  ages  12  to  17, 

97 


referred  by  schools  and  other  agencies  or  people  because  they  were  drop- 
outs  or  were  having  difficulty.  There  was  also  concern  that  these  young 
people  might  present  problems  dining  the  "long  hot  summer." 

The  active  program  was  well  attended.  In  addition  to  group  meetings 
and  home  visits,  the  program  took  children  horseback  riding,  to  Detroit 
Lions'  practice,  to  the  Tigers'  games,  and  to  many  other  interesting  places 
in  the  area.  Street  workers  and  teachers  made  up  most  of  the  program 
staff.  The  youngsteis  were  asked  to  write  their  evaluation  of  the  program, 
too,  and  were  positive  about  the  experience.  Again  with  this  program,as 
with  the  one  just  described,  the  experience  of  the  parent  agencies  was 
important,  and  the  woiker's  skills  and  orientation  were  of  vital  impor- 
tance to  the  success.  The  weakest  part  of  the  program  is  its  temporary  ami 
limited  nature.  When  summer  ends,  the  stresses  and  deprivations  continue, 
and  young  people  live  with  and  react  to  them  all  year.  The  choice  hcicis 
between  a  siiccessful  fragment  or  nothing  at  all. 

Los  Xflge/as-ESCAPE  STRING  (Education  in  Service  Careers  and  Hm- 
ployment-and  Service  Toward  Redirection  of  Impressionable 
Neighborhood  Groups) 

A  major  objective  is  to  help  predelinquent  youth,  and  another  is  to  help 
low-income  Negro  and  Mexican-American  youth  of  academic  promise  to 
continue  their  education  with  the  help  of  salaries  paid  for  work  with  this 
agency.  A  team  of  professionals  contacted  gangs  and  their  families,  and 
then  the  college  students  from  similar  low-income  families  were  assigned 
to  work  with  them.  Professionals  supervised  the  college  students,  A  very 
satisfactory  aspect  was  the  involvement  of  the  students  in  the  agency.  As 
the  program  went  on,  if  any  of  the  college  students  were  interested  In 
social  work,  everything  possible  was  done  to  assist  them  in  attending  a 
graduate  school  of  social  work.  The  staff  felt  that  in  some  cases  tlie 
experience  gave  some  college  students  the  motivation  to  stay  in  college. 
The  young  people  helped  also  benefited.  Again,  the  long  experience  in  this 
field  of  the  parent  agency  was  important. 

Los  Angeles  had  many  other  programs: 

The  South  Central  Volunteer  Bureau  recruited  volunteers  for  work  in 
ghetto  areas.  It  was  very  active  in  helping  get  food  and  supplies  into  the 
Watts  area  during  the  riot.  It  was  felt  that  many  volunteers  benefited  from 
this  experience. 

The  Neighborhood  Adult  Participation  Aides  worked  with  a  wide 
variety  of  agencies  to  allow  them  to  extend  their  services  into  areas  thai 
normally  would  not  be  offered.  Homemaking  services  or  foster  father 
activities  were  two  examples. 

The  Los  Angeles  Human  Relations  Commission  had  a  number  of 
projects.  They  hired  some  militant  young  Negroes  to  be  a  link  between 
them  and  the  community.  It  was  felt  that  this  helped  in  preventing 
trouble  in  their  areas. 

The  Los  Angeles  County  Civil  Service  instituted  a  program  to  employ 
poverty  youth  in  30  types  of  civil  service  jobs  as  aides.  They  also  searched 

98 


for  other  types  of  employment.  Testing  and  orientation  were  given  prior 
to  employment.  Role  playing  and  movies  were  used  to  help  educate  and 
prepare  the  young  people.  Psychiatric  consultation  was  used  when  neces- 
sary and  monthly  employment  followup  was  done  where  necessary. 

Travelers  Aid  gave  special  help  to  newcomeis  to  the  Watts  area.  Money 
was  available  to  help  with  interim  crises.  Public  assistance  was  not 
available  here  until  residency  requirements  were  met.  Help  was  given  with 
school  problems,  medical  care,  legal  difficulties,  mental  illness,  etc.  Aides 
from  the  previously  mentioned  Neighborhood  Adult  Participation  pro- 
gram weie  extensively  used. 

Rochester,  New  Yoik  State  Division  of  Employment:  Special  Youth 
Project;  Manpower;  and  City-County  Youth  Board:  Youth  and  Work 
Project. 

The  focus  for  these  three  related  programs  was  employment  of 
poverty-level  youth  and  appropriate  training  and  job  experience  for  them. 
The  State  Employment  Service  had  "outreach"  programs  to  recruit  youth 
in  streets,  bars,  pool  halls,  etc.  Some  workcis  could  speak  Spanish  so  that 
Cubans  and  Puerto  Ricans  could  be  reached.  Contacts  were  followed  up  if 
office  appointments  were  not  kept. 

The  Manpower  Section  was  a  training  piogram  run  by  the  Board  of 
Education  and  the  Employment  Service.  It  offered  remedial  programs, 
counseling,  and  primarily  vocational  training. 

The  Youth  and  Work  Program  was  for  girls  and  boys  16-18  years  old. 
Those  who  were  least  likely  to  find  satisfactory  jobs  and  most  likely  to  get 
into  trouble  were  selected.  Odd  jobs  in  the  program  office  were  used  for 
diagnostic  purposes.  Then  some  of  the  young  people  were  given  individual 
assignments.  Others  were  given  group  assignments  with  group  discussion 
first,  and  then  were  interviewed  for  jobs  or  received  more  training. 

Many  other  programs  were  examined  during  this  study.  One  conclusion 
was  that  the  experience  of  local  groups  well  grounded  in  work  with  this 
population  was  very  important.  The  locally  shaped  programs  were  charac- 
terized by  flexibility  and  imaginative  use  of  existing  agencies  and  facilities. 
It  was  a  general  consensus  that  moiale  was  high  on  these  programs  and  the 
most  discouraging  feature  was  their  time-limited  existence  in  some  cases. 
These  programs  were,  by  and  large,  well  able  to  attract  the  number  of 
young  people  they  were  designed  to  serve.  Young  people  did  want  jobs 
and  training.  The  participants  felt  that  the  jobs  they  obtained  had  the 
value  of  being  hopeful  examples  for  others  in  their  neighborhoods. 

However,  these  piograms,  it  was  stressed  by  those  interviewed,  did  not 
basically  change  the  ghetto.  The  stresses  that  unemployment  cause,  and 
the  increasing  problems  caused  by  automation,  would  take  far  larger  and 
more  potent  projects  to  combat. 

Racial  Feelings 

This  study  discloses  the  feelings  that  have  become  more  pronounced  as 
time  has  passed.  At  the  time  of  this  study  the  issue  of  Black  Identity  and 

99 


Black  Pride  was  well  in  evidence  in  the  ghetto.  The  study  showed  that 
despite  the  riots  of  that  period  the  anti-white  feeling  was  not  as  high  as 
might  have  been  expected.  The  Black  Militants  influenced  directly  only  a 
small  part  of  the  ghetto  population,  although  the  climate  of  hate  thai  tlicy 
preached  was  moving  in  that  direction,  An  awakening  of  many  young 
people  to  the  racial  inequities  was  described.  Even  the  most  successful 
young  people  in  our  city  slums  are  finding  that  they  cannot  love  anyone 
who  helps  to  keep  them  down.  The  anger  is  high  and  the  chances  that  it 
will  erupt  against  a  policeman  or  some  other  symbol  of  their  oppression  is 
very  great. 

The  study  showed  that  a  key  problem  is  in  their  own  identity.  The 
tremendous  necessity  to  reach  back  and  find  a  cultural  history  that  can 
bolster  respect  has  led  many  to  study  Africa.  Others  are  searching  for 
Negro  achievements  in  more  recent  slavery  and  post-slavery  periods.  A 
difficulty  here  is  that  many  Negroes  who  are  tremendously  accomplished 
have  achieved  this  by  "doing  it  Whitey's  way"  which  causes  many  Negroes 
to  classify  them  as  "Uncle  Toms."  The  solid  achievement  of  even  ac- 
complishing that  is  often  Iqst  in  this  issue.  Despite  the  fact  that  this  stud/ 
showed  that  many  "successful"  Negroes  who  have  achieved  middle-class 
status  are  very  interested  and  concerned  about  what  happens  in  Die 
ghetto,  this  concern  is  not  believed  or  recognized  for  the  most  part.  Un- 
doubtedly this  is  one  of  the  reasons  that  the  civil  rights  groups  are  unable 
to  enlist  more  basic  support.  The  Urban  League  and  the  NAACP  are  often 
characterized  as  "Uncle  Tom"  associations, 

Mexican-Americans  and  Puerto  Ricans  were  beginning  in  1 965  to  show 
signs  that  they,  too,  would  organize  and  ferment  to  achieve  more  oppor- 
tunity. In  some  parts  of  the  country  their  conditions  are  the  worst 
possible.  A  particular  problem  is  the  language  issue.  Schools  have  been 
unable  or  unwilling  to  fully  meet  the  complication  of  having  Spanish  be  a 
primary  language  for  this  group  and  still  helping  them  to  learn  the  neces- 
sary English  to  compete  successfully  here.  This  is  a  major  problem.  In 
1966  there  were  about  eight  hundred  thousand  Mexican-Americans  in  Los 
Angeles  County  alone.  The  struggle  of  the  National  Farm  Workers'  Asso- 
ciation to  get  higher  wages  and  better  conditions  for  grape  workers  was 
beginning  at  the  time  of  this  study.  It  has  subsequently  become  more  of  a 
political  issue.  In  1 968  one  California  congressman  passed  out  grapes  with 
a  sign  that  indicated  one  should  enjoy  the  "forbidden  fruit,"  and  fie 
received  many  packages  back  from  congressmen  in  other  States  who  did 
not  wish  to  anger  constituents  or  labor  groups  by  being  seen  to  accept 
them. 

One  Mexican-American  group  of  about  50  members  walked  out  of  a 
Federal  Equal  Opportunities  Conference  because  they  were  not  repre- 
sented on  the  commission  that  planned  the  conference.  A  large  group  held 
a  banquet  to  celebrate  this  act, 

Though  in  some  ways  the  problems  resemble  those  of  the  Negroes, 
there  is  a  more  marked  resemblance  to  the  patterns  experienced  by  other 
immigrant  groups.  This  study  and  the  one  in  1963  showed  that  the  youth 
seem  to  be  between  two  cultures.  It  was  the  impression  at  this  time  that 

100 


they  felt  superior  to  the  Mexicans  and  inferior  to  the  Anglos.  This  causes 
the  classic  pattern  of  family  tension  and  lack  of  respect  for  the  parents 
and  the  old  ways.  Gang  formation  is  a  common  way  to  deal  with  this  and 
was  attractive  to  those  unfortunate  enough  to  lemain  in  the  slums. 

The  Puerto  Ricans  are  in  a  similar  but  different  situation.  In  New  York 
in  1 964,  figures  show  that  Puerto  Ricans  were  even  poorer  as  a  group  than 
Negroes.  They  share  with  Negroes  high  rates  of  public  assistance  cases, 
large  families,  high  unemployment,  excessive  interest  lates  for  loans,  etc. 
They  share  language  and  cultural  tension  with  the  Mexican-Americans. 
They,  however,  seem  to  have  considerable  pride  in  being  from  Puerto  Rico 
and  tills  is  a  definite  plus,  just  as  the  island  is  a  retreat  and  a  refuge  for 
them  from  life  on  the  mainland. 

The  Puerto  Rican  Community  Development  Project  is  a  comprehensive 
plan  and  program  for  meeting  a  wide  range  of  needs.  It  was  financed  by 
antipoverty  funds  and  was  built  upon  existing  programs. 


Conclusions 

By  1965  the  apathy  had  coalesced  into  anger.  The  conditions  in  the 
slums  and  the  state  of  day-to-day  life  proved  too  provocative  and  unyield- 
ing. The  methods  of  protest  were  too  tame  and  unsatisfying  to  the  needs 
of  the  ghetto  resident.  The  alienated  young  person  living  in  the  slums  and 
feeling  he  had  so  little  to  lose  was  immersed  in  this  atmosphere.  The 
release  felt  by  many  who  had  been  pent  up  was  stronger  than  the  prohi- 
bitions. Although  mixed  feelings  about  the  riots  are  common,  the  pride  at 
turning  their  image  from  "shiftless"  to  dangerous  was  welcome  for  many 
Negroes.  It  is  definitely  more  manly  to  be  dangerous  in  this  country  than 
it  is  to  be  "shiftless  and  lazy."  Whereas  the  civil  rights  movement  was  able 
to  achieve  change  in  the  South  and  the  new  laws  were  making  changes,  the 
latter  were  not  of  sufficient  measure  to  make  a  difference  in  the  ghettos 
of  the  North.  The  riots  undoubtedly  hurt  the  Negro  communities 
involved-they  have  suffered  real  loss,  food  scarcity,  and  loss  of  sustaining 
businesses.  But  the  gains  are  real  also  and  unfortunately  are  often  more 
visible  than  the  gains  of  slower,  more  moderate  methods.  Politicians  and 
policemen,  for  instance,  have  become  more  conscious  of  ghetto  problems 
and  their  explosive  potential. 

Whites,  too,  have  suffered.  All  have  suffered.  The  investigator  found 
that  during  this  study  it  was  virtually  impossible  to  limit  the  time  spent  on 
this  subject  with  any  respondent  as  the  feelings  flowed  in  such  an  intense 
and  meaningful  way. 

A  warning  has  come  out  of  this  study  not  to  lump  minority  groups 
together.  Specificity  and  tailoring  to  meet  conditions  in  individual  areas 
have  been  successful.  Other  suggestions  are  that  intergroup  contact  should 
be  fostered.  Conference,  tutoring  programs,  training  programs  are  all 
valuable  if  they  contain  meaningful  contact  between  ethnic  groups.  Goals 
which  are  yielding  of  fairly  quick  success  win  alienated  young  people  into 
organized  campaigns  faster  than  more  abstract  goals. 

101 


The  study  suggests  that  the  formation  of  overall  city  poverty  agencies 
may  be  unwise.  It  was  found  that  they  tend  to  generate  problems  that  get 
in  the  way  of  their  own  work.  The  investigator  suggests  that  perhaps  it  is 
better  foi  local  agencies  to  deal  directly  with  Federal  officials  foi  anti- 
poverty  funds.  Another  point  is  that  funding  is  important  to  agencies  and 
must  be  given  a  firm  enough  and  long  term  enough  basis  to  make  it  viable, 
This  study  revealed  that  many  agencies  are  forced  to  cm  tail  and  limit  tncii 
involvements  due  to  fear  of  cutbacks  or  loss  of  funds.  There  is  the  concern 
that  programs  begun  and  dropped  can  arouse  more  antagonism  than  those 
never  stalled.  Many  agencies  have  to  devote  themselves  mainly  to  keeping 
alive. 

Further,  the  investigator  urges  that  the  real  serious  nature  of  the 
problem  of  the  ghettos  be  faced.  He  recognizes  that  a  realistic  appniisuf  of 
economic  and  social  network  that  causes  slums  and  its  tremendous  human 
toll  be  faced.  Varied  programs  are  needed.  This  study  clearly  shows  that 
no  one  type  of  program  will  reach  all.  Even  if  at  times  the  cost  may  be 
high,  such  programs  must  still  be  supported.  The  difficulty  and,  :it  the 
same  time,  the  ease  of  predicting  riots  is  thought-provoking.  In  1 969  some 
politicians  and  civil  rights  leaders  are  predicting  that  our  major  riots  are 
over,  but  this  is  hardly  assured,  and,  if  it  were,  the  human  cost  of  our 
racial  problems  and  the  loss  of  any  of  our  young  people  would  still  be  loo 
high.  The  commitment  has  been  made  in  this  country  many  times  and  in 
many  ways  and  needs  now  to  be  made  a  reality. 

The  investigator  suggests  that  many  legislative  and  other  steps  arc 
needed  to  combat  poverty.  The  guaranteed  annual  income  is  suggested  as 
one  idea  that  might  be  successful.  Cooperative  housing  might  overcome 
the  need  for  large  investments  on  the  pait  of  poor  people  and  give  them  a 
chance  at  ownership  lather  than  at  being  renters.  Street  work  and  social 
agencies  have  a  strong  chance  at  helping  touch  alienated  youth.  The  pride 
in  racial  identity  needs  to  be  supported,  but  not  the  hate  messages  tlial 
sometimes  accompany  it.  Indigenous  woikers  reciuited  from  an  area  to 
serve  an  area  are  also  particularly  successful,  and  these  positions  provide 
employment  for  those  who  might  otherwise  be  unemployed,  which  is  also 
valuable.  Birth  control  information  is  often  sought  in  the  slums  anil 
should  be  made  available.  The  cities  need  to  improve  on  services  to  Die 
deprived  areas  and  so  on.  Primarily,  tolerance  must  be  found  for  Ik 
forward  and  then  backward  and  forward  again  patterns  of  work  in  this 
area. 

This  study  suggests  that  education  in  the  slums  is  a  complex  issue  and  an 
important  one.  While  the  need  for  technical  education  exists  and  is  impor- 
tant in  preparing  young  people  for  our  mechanized  society,  the  as- 
sumption should  not  be  made  that  this  type  of  education  is  all  that  is 
needed  in  the  slums.  Many  students  need  assistance,  educationally  and 
financially,  to  achieve  their  potential.  Many  more  could  attend  college. 
Programs  with  liberal  admission  policies,  even  pteferentia!  admissions, 
etc.,  need  to  be  developed. 

102 


The  investigator  also  postulates  that,  if  each  large  firm  undertook  to 
hire  and  train  some  of  these  depiived  young  people,  the  effect  would  be 
striking. 

As  has  been  previously  mentioned,  community  service  positions 
utilizing  indigenous  workers  can  fill  some  important  needs  in  oui  society. 
Almost  all  large  community  institutions,  such  as  hospitals,  schools,  and 
social  agencies  need  help.  The  presently  alienated,  almost  wasted  young 
peison  could  provide  the  manpower  needed  for  better  operation  in  all 
these  community  institutions.  Training  is  the  key  in  both  private  and 
government  programs  for  effectively  teaching  and  employing  these  young 
people.  ' 

A  major  mistake  would  be  to  underestimate  the  forces  that  keep  slums 
as  slums  and  keep  minority  groups  in  them.  The  solutions  to  these  prob- 
lems are  extremely  complex.  One  conviction  of  the  investigator  is  that  the 
poor  should  be  consulted  and  involved  with  progiams,  even  though  this 
may  at  times  make  progress  slower  and  more  complicated. 

Research  Grant.  Mil  11396 

Dates  of  Intel  view:  September  1968 

References: 

Dems  tern,  S.  Youth  on  the  Streets,  Work  with  Alienated  Youth  Gtoups.  New  York: 

Association  Press,  1964.  160pp. 
Alternatives  to  Violence,  Alienated  Youth  and  Riots,  Race  and  Poverty.  New  York; 

Association  Press,  1967.  192  pp. 
Schulberg,  B.  Fiom  the  Ashes.  New  Yoik:  New  American  Library,  1967.  275  pp. 


103 


Project  Director! 

Reginalds.  Lourie,  M.D, 
Children's  Hospital 
Washington,  D.C. 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 

A  pilot  project  for  training  a  new  type  of  mental  health  worker,  ji  child 
development  counselor  who  would  make  a  frontline  effort  to  intcrcep! 
emotional  disability  before  it  could  take  hold,  is  now  complete  and  the 
results  are  being  evaluated.  Because  the  lessons  being  drawn  should  be 
useful  in  other  programs  to  develop  unconventional  manpower  soutces. 

The  reasoning  behind  the  project  ran  like  this: 

In  the  typical  well-baby  clinic,  the  pediatrician  and  the  nurse  can  give 
only  a  short  time  to  each  mother  and  child.  Tins  has  generally  proved 
sufficient  for  noting  signs  of  physical  illness  and  of  irregularities  in 
physical  development  and  for  being  concerned  with  preventive  approaches 
in  general.  But  it  is  hardly  sufficient  in  most  clinics  for  noting  and 
counseling  about  signs  of  emotional  maladjustment  in  the  child  or  of 
maternal  attitudes  likely  to  lead  to  such  maladjustment.  If  the  clinic  staff 
could  include  a  person  whose  main  concern  was  the  normal  emotional  and 
psychological  development  of  the  child,  the  clinic  might  be  better  able  (o 
head  off  crippling  problems.  Well-baby  clinics,  in  short,  might  better  fulfill 
their  mission  of  bolstering  the  mental  as  well  as  the  physical  well-being  of 
the  children  passing  through  them. 

Since  the  traditional  mental  health  disciplines-including  medicine, 
psychology,  nursing,  and  social  work-cannot  meet  even  the  existing 
demands  on  them,  however,  where  are  these  new  workers  to  come  from? 

An  earlier  pilot  project  conceived  by  Dr.  Margaret  Rioch,  a  psycho- 
logist, had  found  a  new  manpower  supply  in  mature,  intelligent  mothers 
with  a  keen  interest  in  the  community  and  a  desire  to  serve.  The  piojcct 
had  demonstrated  that  such  women  could  be  trained  for  counseling 
adolescents  and  adults  in  mental  health  clinics  and.other  centers,  and  tlint 
a  variety  of  agencies  and  institutions  were  eager  to  employ  them. 

Wouldn't  it  be  possible  to  take  another  group  of  such  women  and  tiain 
them-again  over  a  2-year  period,  half  time-for  a  new  profession,  child 
development  counseling?  And  couldn't  they  then  contribute  significantly 
to  a  highly  important  but  relatively  neglected  part  of  the  mental  health 
campaign-preventive  approaches  in  the  earliest  years  of  life? 

104 


To  answei  such  questions,  Dr.  Reginald  S.  Loune,  chief  of  the  depart- 
ment of  psychiatry  of  the  Children's  Hospital,  Washington,  D.C,  and 
medical  director  of  the  affiliated  Hillcrest  Children's  Center,  undertook  to 
direct  an  Institute-financed  tiaining  program  for  child  development 
counselors.  The  project  was  sponsored  both  by  the  research  foundation  of 
the  hospital  and  by  the  Bureau  of  Maternal  and  Child  Health  of  the 
District  of  Columbia's  Department  of  Public  Health.  Dr.  Samuel  Schwartz, 
the  Bureau's  chief,  was  codirecloi  with  Dr.  Lourie.  Through  most  of  the 
project,  Dr.  Rioch,  who  had  diiected  the  earlier  program  and  stimulated 
the  thinking  in  this  one,  served  as  training  director,  and  Margaiet  Stolzen- 
bach,  a  graduate  of  that  program,  was  her  executive  assistant  and 
coordinator.  The  evaluation  phase  is  being  conducted  by  Dr.  Stuart  E. 
Golann,  of  the  University  of  Maryland  and  the  American  Psychological 
Association. 

The  tiaining  centers  were,  in  the  main,  the  well-baby  clinics  of  the 
District  of  Columbia  Department  of  Public  Health  and  the  one  at  the 
hospital.  The  project  directors  felt,  however,  that  child  development 
counselors  probably  could  be  highly  useful  at  nurseiy  schools,  day-care 
centers,  and  almost  anywhere  else  that  mothers  and  young  children  are 
found,  so  the  trainees  were  made  acquainted  with  a  variety  of  institutions. 
Most  of  the  instruction  was  carried  on  in  the  pediatric  and  psychiatric 
facilities  of  the  hospital.  The  teachers  included  two  dozen  authorities- 
many  from  the  staffs  of  the  sponsoring  institutions  but  a  number  from 
outside— in  such  fields  as  psychiatry,  psychology,  child  development, 
family  and  child  therapy,  social  anthropology,  and  psychiatric  social 
work. 


The  Trainees  and  the  Program 

To  recruit  trainees,  staff  members  discussed  the  project's  hopes  and 
plans  with  each  of  about  50  persons  who  were  leaders  in  the  community 
or  represented  such  organizations  as  PTA's,  women's  clubs,  community 
service  agencies,  and  college  alumnae  groups.  This  was  done  by  telephone. 
Descriptions  of  the  projects  were  then  mailed  to  interested  organizations 
so  they  could  be  posted,  read  at  meetings,  or  published  in  newsletters. 
Stipends  of  $1,000  per  year  were  offered  trainees.  Women  requesting 
application  blanks  were  given  a  preliminary  telephone  interview  that 
served,  among  othei  purposes,  to  correct  misunderstandings  about  the 
project  and  to  emphasize  the  uncertain  future  of  applicants  selected  for 
training. 

Out  of  the  101  women  who  completed  their  applications,  eight  were 
finally  selected.  All  eight -on  the  basis  of  autobiographies,  interviews,  and 
group  discussions  with  members  of  the  training  staff-rated  high  in  intel- 
ligence, perceptiveness,  self-awareness,  integrity,  and  emotional  maturity. 
Says  Dr.  Lourie;  "We  ended  with  bright,  warm,  flexible,  empathic,  verbal 
women  who  were  interested  in  others  and  intellectually  curious." 

The  women  chosen  ranged  in  age  from  about  35  to  about  50.  All  but 
one  were  white,  though  it  had  been  hoped  that  about  half  would  be 

105 


Negro,  because  in  Washington  the  well-baby  clinics  serve  a  primarily  I 
population.  (A  second  Negro  woman  had  been  recruited,  but  she 
drew  before  the  training  began.)  All  were  mothers,  though  their  chi. 
were  either  grown  or  well  along  in  school,  and  all  were  paiticipath 
community  activities.  Five  were  college  graduates,  including  one  wo 
with  a  master's  degree,  and  the  otheis  had  had  at  least  2  years  of  col 
Most  of  the  husbands  were  professional  people. 

Preceding  the  start  of  training,  in  February  1964,  the  stuff  n 
arrangements  with  American  University  to  grant  credits  toward  citli- 
bachelor's  or  a  master's  degree.  It  was  reasoned  that  the  counselors  wo 
always  be  woiking  alongside  professional  people  and  that  (hi 
presumably,  would  be  interested  in  their  academic  backgrounds.  Also 
was  hoped,  academic  recognition  for  a  new  type  of  health-field  prof 
sional  might  eventually  be  developed. 

The  training  piogram  was  divided  into  four  semesters  which  wi 
spread,  college-style,  over  a  2-year  period.  The  trainees  worked  half  tttu 
They  had  the  following  weekly  schedule  during  the  first  semester: 

•  Three   hours  of  lectures   on  the  physiological,  sociological,  an 
psychological  aspects  of  child  development. 

•  A  2-hour  seminar  on  personality  development. 

•  Four  hours  of  practical  classioom  work  dealing  with  clinical  tfls 
histories-from  well-baby  and  other  types  of  clinics-in  order  to  learn  Hit 
kinds  of  problems  that  arise  and  how  to  handle  them. 

t  Half  a  day  at  a  well-baby  clinic,  generally  one  of  the  12  opera  ted  by  the 
District  of  Columbia  Department  of  Public  Health  and  known  officially  as 
Maternal  and  Child  Health  Clinics,  For  two  weeks  the  trainees  observed 
clinical  routine  by  accompanying  an  assigned  family  through  all  the  clinic 
procedures,  including  a  home  visit  by  a  public  health  nurse.  Then  for  (wo 
weeks  they  took  routine  case  histories  of  mothers  selected  by  the  clinic 
staff.  After  that,  clinic  staffs  were  asked  to  refer  to  trainees  those  mothers 
who  expressed  interest  in  discussing  problems  at  greater  length  than  stuff- 
time  permitted.  The  trainees  were  urged  to  invite  additional  interviews 
with  these  mothers  and  to  spend  unscheduled  time  in  informal  waiting 
room  contacts. 

•  An  hom  of  counseling,  at  a  well-baby  clinic,  under  the  Jmmcdinic 
supervision  of  a  member  of  the  training  program's  staff. 

•  A  conference  with  staff  members,  primarily  to  give  the  trainees  nn 
opportunity  to  discuss  their  work  and  to  ask  questions  and  to  give  the 
staff  an  opportunity  to  gauge  progress. 

For  the  second  and  third  semesters,  the  fieldwork  was  expanded  to 
include  one  full  day  a  week  at  a  well-baby  clinic  and  half  a  day  In  a 
suburban  nursery  school,  where  the  trainees  could  observe  the  behavior  of 
children-and  the  attitudes  of  their  mothers-who  were  growing  up  to  a 
privileged  section  of  the  metropolitan  area.  The  trainees  also  visited  a 
variety  of  other  agencies  to  learn  the  kinds  of  helping  resources  available 
and  the  problems  facing  them.  Lectures  and  observational  work  during 
the  second  semester  dealt  with  the  following  topics,  among  others: 
psychological  and  psychiatric  examinations,  pediatric  consultation,  the 

306 


child's  early  development,  mental  retardation,  speech  and  hearing, 
nutrition,  nursery  school  orientation,  child  rearing,  and  planned  parent- 
hood. Classroom  work  during  the  third  semester  included  4  hours  of 
lecture-discussion  seminais  covering  child-rearing  practices,  family  inter- 
action, psychodynamics,  the  psychosocial  effects  of  illness,  and  tech- 
niques of  parent-teacher  consultation. 

During  the  fourth  and  last  semester,  six  of  the  trainees  continued  to 
spend  a  day  a  week  at  a  well-baby  clinic,  while  the  other  two  worked  in  a 
maternity  clinic,  supervised  by  an  obstetrician.  The  trainees  also  spent  one 
day  a  week  working  with  one  of  a  number  of  other  organizations- 
neighborhood  centers,  a  mental  health  clinic,  the  Family  and  Child  Service 
Agency,  the  District  of  Columbia  Day  Care  Association,  the  Jewish  Social 
Service  Agency,  and  the  Prince  Geoiges  County,  Md.,  Maternal  and  Child 
Health  Clinic, 

Course  work  during  this  final  term  included  3  hours  a  week  on  "The 
Process  of  Child  Development."  The  first  half  of  the  couise  was  given  in 
the  maternity  wards  at  D.C.  General  Hospital,  where  trainees  observed 
newborn  babies,  talked  with  the  mothers,  and  participated  in  a  seminar 
with  nurses  assigned  to  the  maternity  section.  Later,  at  the  Children's 
Hospital,  children  up  to  6  months  of  age  were  observed  intensively.  Then 
children  between  the  ages  of  I  and  2  years  were  studied,  Ward  observa- 
tions were  supplemented  by  lectures  and  films. 

Among  their  other  activities,  the  tiainees  attended  a  course  on  adoles- 
cence given  at  Howard  University  School  of  Social  Work;  sat  in  on 
diagnostic  sessions  at  the  Department  of  Psychiatry  of  the  Children's 
Hospital;  attended  lectures  in  the  pediatric-psychiatric  training  program  of 
the  same  hospital;  and  observed  a  number  of  interviews  between  therapists 
and  patients  at  the  Clinical  Center,  National  Institutes  of  Health. 


Major  Problems  Encountered 

In  the  disadvantagcd  areas  served  by  most  of  the  Washington  well-baby 
stations,  many  of  the  mothers  were  uninterested  in  talking  with  the 
trainees.  They  wanted  mainly  to  get  Ihiough  with  the  pediatrician  and  the 
nurse  and  to  get  away  from  the  usually  crowded  clinic.  The  many  who  did 
want  to  talk  generally  presented  urgent  problems  of  their  own-the  need 
for  a  place  to  live,  for  clothes,  for  legal  aid  in  marital  problems,  for  help 
with  a  delinquent  older  child. 

The  trainees  felt  obliged  to  offer  this  pressingly  needed  assistance, 
usually  with  the  aid  of  community  agencies  the  mothers  had  not  known 
about  or  had  been  afraid  to  try,  because  it  was  the  humane  thing  to  do.  In 
addition,  particularly  during  the  early  part  of  the  training,  when  the 
women  often  wondered  if  they  were  being  really  useful,  it  gave  them  a 
sense  of  accomplishment.  Sometimes  the  help  with  the  urgent  practical 
problems  opened  the  way  to  do  something  about  the  basic  principles  of 
preventive  mental  health  work  centered  on  the  child-to  try  to  get  across 
the  idea,  notably,  that  the  way  a  youngster  is  tended,  talked  to,  and 
played  with  will  affect  his  future  emotional  well-being  and  his  ability  to 

107 


learn.  Generally  such  efforts  had  to  be  neglected  or  abandoned,  howei 
because  the  mothers  visited  the  clinics  infrequently.  A  few  did  maker 
keep  special  appointments  with  the  trainees. 

During  the  latter  part  of  the  training  program,  the  counselors  rcpori 
greater  success  in  directing  the  interviews  to  the  field  of  child  devel< 
ment  and  in  dealing  with  problems  directly  concerned  with  the  chilrtn 
Included  were  problems  of  nutrition,  shyness,  destructive  bchavh 
nervous  habits,  and  slowness.  Information  that  the  mothers  would  n 
otherwise  have  had  on  such  matters  was  made  available.  A  report  on  ( 
project's  fourth  semester  notes  that  "there  seemed  to  be  more  counseli 
and  less  social  work," 

During  this  semester,  in  the  couise  of  their  work  at  the  well-baby  aj 
maternity  clinics,  the  trainees  talked  with  450  mothers  fora  total  of 6( 
interviews.  This  was  an  average  of  about  83  interviews  per  trainee,  and  < 
about  six  per  working  day  at  the  clinic.  Most  of  the  interviews  Instc 
between  15  and  40  minutes,  but  some  ran  more  than  an  hour,  Aboi 
three-fourths  of  the  mothers  interviewed  had  been  referred  by  the  cliiu 
doctor;  most  of  the  otheis  had  been  approached  directly  by  the  trainee  i 
the  clinic  waiting  room.  Some  300  of  the  mothers  were  seen  only  once. 

Dr.  Lourie  points  out  that  even  when  the  main  concern  had  to  be  til 
compelling  problems  of  everyday  life,  the  trainees  were  actually  doin; 
preventive  work  with  the  child.  For  if  they  could  help  take  care  of  lfn 
mother's  needs,  she  in  turn  would  take  better  care  of  the  baby's.  The  goa 
of  the  training  program,  though,  had  been  different.  In  short,  the  well 
baby  clinics,  with  only  sporadic  family  contact  available,  proved  less  than 
ideal  for  providing  the  counselors-in-training  with  experience  in  the  kind 
of  preventive  counseling  it  had  been  envisaged  they  would  do  and  for 
which  their  other  work  was  preparing  them, 

A  second  important  problem  had  to  do  with  the  attitudes  of  the  other 
people  working  in  the  clinics.  The  Department  of  Public  Health  had  joined 
with  the  project's  training  staff  to  explain  to  the  clinic  personnel  lite 
concept  of  preventive  work  by  a  child-development  counselor.  The  results 
varied  considerably,  the  project  reports,  ranging  from  eager  acceptance  al 
one  end  to  open  resistance  at  the  other,  with  indifference  in  the  middle. 
Resistance  and  noncooperation  tend  to  be  met  whenener  a  new  group  of 
"manpower  multipliers"  is  proposed  for  the  health  field,  Dr.  Lourie  notes. 
The  fundamental  cause  is  the  tendency  of  professional  workers  (o  view 
the  new  group  as  a  threat  to  the  standards  of  their  profession-die 
academic  degree,  the  length  of  training,  the  other  requisites  they  them- 
selves have  met. 

All  the  trainees  reported  they  could  work  effectively  only  when  [lie 
clinic  pediatrician-and  preferably  the  nurse  also -understood  and 
accepted  the  program's  aims,  was  willing  to  make  referrals  to  tin 
counselor,  and  could  give  her  consultation  time.  These  requirements  were 
not  always  met,  particularly  in  the  early  part  of  the  program. 

As  trainees  and  clinic  staff  got  to  know  one  another,  the  resistance 
generally  gave  way  to  cooperation.  Three  pediatricians  became  especially 
interested  in  the  program,  so  the  well-baby  clinics  they  directed  were  tlie 

108 


ones  used  during  the  fourth  semester.  At  the  end  of  this  term  they  and  the 
director  of  the  maternity  clinic  were  asked  to  rate  the  counseling  service 
from  the  standpoint  of  usefulness;  all  rated  it  as  significant  or  potentially 
significant.  They  were  also  asked  to  rate  the  individual  counselors  on  such 
items  as  ability  to  observe  child  with  discretion,  understanding  of  child, 
ability  to  function  usefully  with  patients  as  individuals,  and  behavior  as  a 
professional  person.  Out  of  a  total  of  48  ratings  for  the  eight  trainees, 
only  three  ratings  fell  as  low  as  satisfactory;  all  the  others  were  good  or 
excellent 

Perhaps  significantly,  it  was  not  until  this  last  semester  that  the 
pediatricians  still  in  the  program  felt  able  to  score  all  the  trainees  on  all 
the  individual  items. 


The  Post-Training  Experience 

Contrary  to  expectations,  the  District's  well-baby  clinics  had  no  jobs 
for  the  counselors  at  the  end  of  the  training  period,  in  January  1966. 
Project  officials  explain  that  in  spite  of  improved  relationships,  there  were 
people  at  administrative  levels  not  yet  ready  to  accept  the  concept  of 
counselors  trained  as  these  had  been.  A  defensive  attitude  by  the 
traditional  professions  may  be  only  part  of  the  explanation,  these  officials 
think;  another  part  may  be  a  conviction  that  the  preventive  approach  to 
mental  illness  is  impractical— as  it  often  seemed  to  be,  in  the  atmosphere 
of  the  clinics,  even  to  the  trainees  themselves. 

Seven  of  the  graduates  took  half-time  positions  with  another  Health 
Department  activity,  a  community  mental  health  program,  and  the  eighth 
went  to  work  for  the  well-baby  clinic  at  the  Children's  Hospital.  Several  of 
the  half-time  workers  took  on  another  part-time  job-with  a  mental  health 
clinic,  the  Jewish  Social  Service  Agency,  or  the  National  Capital  Day  Care 
Association. 

The  seven  counselors  in  the  community  mental  health  program  had 
been  hired  to  do  preventive  work,  they  thought,  but  the  pressure  to  help 
the  children  who  were  already  emotionally  ill,  and  their  parents,  was  so 
great  that  preventive  action  had  to  be  postponed.  "It's  like  fire  fighting," 
Dr.  Lourie  remarks,  "If  you  fireproofed  the  buildings,  you  wouldn't  have 
so  many  fires.  But  you  can't  stop  fighting  fires  in  order  to  do  the  fire- 
proofing.  Though  we  did  not  expect  our  trainees  to  have  to  fight  fires, 
they  did  apparently  make  good  emergency  firemen." 

As  part  of  the  project's  evaluation  process,  which  is  continuing,  the 
counselors'  supervisors  and  fellow  workers  were  interviewed  about  3 
months  after  the  graduates  had  been  on  the  job.  Those  interviewed 
included  pediatricians,  psychiatrists,  public  health  doctors,  public  health 
nurses,  social  workers,  and  teachers.  Virtually  all  said  that  they  were 
pleased  with  the  ability  and  the  performance  of  the  new  workers,  and  that 
they  considered  the  women  to  be  examples  of  a  potentially  significant 
manpower  source.  The  counselor  employed  by  the  well-baby  clinic  was 
reported  by  the  clinic's  director  to  have  been  trained  "better  than  the 

109 


pediatrician  to  help  with  the  hundreds  of  patients  who  don't 
psychiatrist."  This  supei visor  also  icported:  "She  does  bcttei  than  you 
doctors.  I'm  continually  delighted  she's  here." 

The  women  themselves  had  a  different  story.  Intei views  and  job  din" 
showed  them  discouraged  and  dissatisfied,  uncertain  both  about  wh 
they  could  do  and  what  they  were  expected  to  do.  The  fiist  fcw  wtvkS' 
months  on  a  job  aie  almost  sure  to  be  an  upsetting  period  for  any  u'tvnti 
trained  person,  Di.  Golann  remarks,  even  if  the  tiainee  has  a  tradition 
degree  and  is  woiking  in  a  chosen,  and  traditional,  field.  But  Ihesi1  cftrf- 
development  counselors  had  been  trained  in  a  new  field,  they  did  ncl 
possess  univeisally  recognized  credentials,  and,  worst,  moat  of  t fit-in  WLVV 
working  in  fields  diffeient  from  the  one  foi  which  they  had  been  lianicJ 

Salaries,  too,  were  a  disappointment.  In  the  eyes  of  graduates  ;m.l  o! 
the  project  staff  as  well,  the  Civil  Service  ranked  the  new  counsolois  Jew 
low.  Further,  it  distinguished  between  those  with  a  college  degree,  wliotf 
it  placed  in  GS-7  classification,  and  those  without  a  degree,  whom  fl 
placed  in  GS-5,  though  the  training  for  child  development  coumt'loi  li;id 
been  the  same  fot  everyone.  This  meant  that  a  counselor  going  to  wwt 
for  a  District  of  Columbia  agency  could  expect  to  start  at  an  annual  salary 
ranging  from  about  $6,500  if  she  had  a  degree  to  about  $5,500  if  slu-diJ 
not. 

The  child  development  counselois  have  dealt  with  their  clissiitisfauta 
in  various  ways: 

•  After  some  months,  two  of  the  eight  left  their  positions  and  yiuolk-d 
at  Howard  Univeisity  to  earn  a  master's  degree  in  social  work,  one  of 
them  studying  full  time  and  the  other  part  time.  They  knew  (hoy  wouM 
have  to  repeat  much  of  the  woik  already  covered,  but,  says  Dr.  Louru1, 
"They  felt  the  need  for  a  label  that  eveiybody  understands.  In  (mining 
new  categoiies  of  manpower  the  identity  problem  is  a  prime  one." 

•  The  counselor  at  the  well-baby  clinic  has  stopped  woiking  in  tliehopf 
of  finding  a  position  in  which  she  feels  more  useful.  In  spite  of  the  praise 
from  the  clinic's  director,  she  didn't  feel  needed. 

•  One  woman  moved  to  New  York  City  with  her  husband  and  lias  token 
a  job  with  an  adoption  agency.  She  counsels  both  the  mothers  who  lime 
adopted  a  child  and  the  natural  mothers. 

•  Three  counselors  are  still  with  the  community  mental  health  program 
but,   having  received  on-the-job  training,   work  mainly   with   troubled 
adults. 

•  The  eighth  counselor  left  her  part-time  job  with  the  mental  health 
program  and  is  employed  full-time  by  the  National  Capital  Day  Care 
Association,  where  she  works  with  mothers,  teachers,  and  children.  Of  all 
*lm"  "  training  program,  she  comes  closest  to  carrying  out  (lie 

-liat  the  counselors  try  to  head  off  potential  problems 

~)r,  Laurie  is  hopeful  that  through  the  work  of 

k>  develop  additional  centers  for  babies  and 

placements    for   some   of   the  other 


Summing  Up 

The  project  has  demonstrated,  members  of  the  tiaining  staff  conclude, 
that  it  is  possible  to  train  mature,  selected  women  in  2  years,  half  time,  to 
do  a  satisfactory  job  as  mental  health  counselois  having  a  specialty  in 
child  development.  At  the  end  of  the  progiam  the  eight  women  could 
counsel  effectively  with  mothers  of  young  children. 

The  project  has  been  less  successful  in  fitting  the  counselois  into  the 
existing  system  of  health  agencies.  By  and  large,  the  trainees  are  doing 
useful  work  in  the  general  field  of  mental  health,  but  it  is  not  the  kind  of 
woik  for  which  they  had  been  trained.  Dr.  Lourie  thinks  this  situation 
may  well  change  as  the  new  community  health  programs  develop  and  new 
centers  for  parents  and  children  evolve  into  developmental  centers. 


Some  of  the  Lessons  Drawn  by  Project  Officials 

Very  early  in  the  planning  of  any  program  looking  to  the  development 
of  a  new  type  of  mental  health  personnel,  the  professional  people- 
including  personnel  officers— of  the  agencies  in  which  the  trainees  will 
work  should  be  drawn  into  it.  They  can  help  ease  the  annoying  everyday 
problems,  such  as  shortage  of  space,  that  arise  when  an  additional  service 
is  injected  into  established  routines.  More  important,  they  can  be  exposed 
to  the  concepts  of  mental  health  counseling  and  to  the  potential  value  of 
the  trainees  in  putting  them  into  practice.  The  understanding  and 
acceptance  of  these  concepts,  even  by  professionally  trained  health  and 
welfare  personnel,  reports  one  member  of  the  project's  staff,  and  the 
willingness  to  cooperate  with  new  types  of  manpower  cannot  be  taken  for 
granted.  Says  another;  "Even  more  time  must  be  spent  educating  the 
existing  professionals  than  the  trainees  themselves."  Included  in  the 
educational  program,  he  reports,  should  be  the  professional  associations 
that  set  the  standaids  for  the  fields  in  which  the  new  counselors  will  be 
working  in  part. 

Dr.  Lourie  adds  that  if  agencies  nnd  institutions  experiencing  manpower 
shortages—shortages  especially  of  intelligent,  sensitive,  mature  people- 
would  analyze  their  needs,  they  would  find  almost  certainly  that  a  large 
part  of  them  could  be  met  by  women  like  those  selected  for  training  in 
the  pilot  project.  "The  question  is  whether  existing  institutions  are 
flexible  enough  to  use  people  like  this  once  they  have  been  trained.  The 
women  in  our  program  are  eager  to  be  useful;  but  they  also  want  to  learn, 
and  they  will  not  stay  long  in  positions  which  do  not  allow  them  to  use 
their  capacities." 

To  insure  the  best  use  of  people  trained  in  nontraditional  ways,  plans 
for  their  employment  should  be  carefully  made  well  in  advance, 
preferably  at  the  same  time  that  the  training  program  itself  is  planned. 

It  is  speculated  that  most  of  the  Negro  women  who  would  have  been 
eligible  for  training  were  already  working  and  could  not  afford  to  under- 
take the  program.  If  more  Negro  women  are  to  be  recruited  for  programs 


such  as  this  one  (and  it  may  very  well  be  that  more  of  the  mothers  at  tlic 
clinics  would  have  sought  the  counsel  of  Negro  trainees),  stipends  Luge 
enough  to  support  them  probably  will  be  necessary,  along  with  a  guaran- 
tee of  employment  at  the  end  of  training. 

Except  on  a  superficial  level  and  with  limited  objectives,  preventive 
mental  health  woik  probably  cannot  be  carried  on  in  well-baby  clinics 
such  as  the  ones,  serving  a  disadvantaged  population,  used  for  the  field- 
training  program  in  Washington.  The  attitudes  and  problems  of  the 
mothers,  along  with  crowded  conditions  and  the  lack  of  cooperation  of 
some  regular  clinic  staff  members,  worked  against  the  Washington  project. 
"This  is  not  to  say  that  the  child  development  counselors  couldn't  and 
didn't  make  a  useful  contribution  there,"  Dr.  Lourie  observes.  "They  were 
trained  hopefully  to  make  a  better  one." 

If  he  were  starting  over  again,  the  director  of  this  pioject  would  stilf 
want  to  train  child  development  counselors,  but  he  would  envisage  putting 
them  to  work  in  a  different  setting-day-care  centers  that  accepted 
children  from  infancy  onward.  He  hopes  such  centers  will  become  part  of 
the  public  school  system.  If  differences  in  learning  abilities,  beyond  (hose 
set  by  heredity,  are  determined  to  a  large  extent  when  a  child  is  vciy 
young,  perhaps  mainly  during  the  first  18  months,  says  Dr.  Lourie,  why 
not  get  the  educators  interested  in  the  child  far  earlier  than  at  present? 
Educators,  welfare  workers,  and  health  people  should  collaborate  in  our 
lowest  socioeconomic  neighborhoods,  he  believes,  to  provide  continuing 
services  from  birth  on  up  into  the  school  years  as  we  now  know  them,  in 
such  a  program  the  child  development  counselor  could  do  her  most 
effective  work. 

Research  Grant:  MH  8322 
Date  of  Interview:  May  18, 1967 


References: 


Golann,  S.  E.  Initial  findings  of  the  follow-up  study  of  child  development  counselors 
1966.  Mimeographed. 

Louire,  R.  S.,  Rioch,  Margaret  J.,  and  Schwartz,  S.  The  concept  of  a  training  program 
for  child  development  counselors.  1 966,  Manuscript. 

Rioch,  Margaret  J.,  Elkes,  Charmian,  and  Flint,  A.  A.  Pilot  project  In  training  menial 
health  counselors.  Public  Health  Service  Publication  No.  1254.  Washington,  Super- 
intendent of  Documents,  U.S.  Government  Printing  Office. 


112 


Director 

A.Paul  Parks 

Operation  Hope 

Los  Angeles,  California 

Prepared  by: 

Gay  Luce 


At  a  time  when  the  voices  of  despair  are  registering  with  ominous 
clarity,  a  small  project  in  Los  Angeles  is  offering  an  antidote  to  the  ugly 
waste  of  young  people  in  urban  ghettos.  Operation  Hope  is  an  experiment- 
al training  program  in  which  20  young  men  and  women  from  Central  and 
East  Los  Angeles  aie  being  helped  through  college,  and  encouraged  to 
acquire  the  skills  and  credentials  that  may  make  them  the  new  leaders  in 
mental  health  professions  such  as  social  work.  The  program  provides  a 
monthly  stipend  of  $200,  the  sine  qua  non,  which  combined  with  part- 
time  job  income,  permits  the  participants  to  study.  Moreover,  the  three 
staff  social  workers,  Director  of  Research,  Administrative  Assistant,  and 
outside  consultants  of  Operation  Hope  provide  special  courses,  legal  and 
psychological  counseling,  and  assistance  during  financial  and  family  crises. 
These  young  people  from  the  ghettos  are  likely  to  revitalize  the  mental 
health  professions,  at  a  time  when  welfare  and  social  workers  are  often 
seen  as  middle-class  visitors,  as  outsiders  who  are  ineffectual  and  hated  by 
ghetto  residents,  and  who  seem  incapable  of  devising  programs  that  will 
attack  the  consequences  of  poverty  and  crowding. 

No  neat  formulae  can  be  offered  for  selecting  and  educating  ghetto 
residents,  but  the  program  evolved  by  Operation  Hope  offers  a  guideline 
that  could  be  adapted  to  any  city  location.  The  impact  of  this  program 
cannot  be  measured  by  statistics,  since  only  a  few  participants  have  been 
funded,  but  the  school  performance  and  careers  of  its  trainees  will  be 
noticed  in  the  next  decade  as  these  people  become  poverty  lawyers, 
teachers,  and  mental  health  workers  in  the  neighborhoods  they  know  well. 


Background 

It  is  shocking  to  discover  that  close  to  one  out  of  every  six  Americans 
lives  in  a  poverty  pocket,  an  island  of  hopelessness  surrounded  by  af- 
fluence. Innercity  ghettos  do  not  communicate  with  the  "outside  world"; 

113 


however,  the  young,  who  have  grown  up  in  an  era  of  television,  have  seen 
the  wideJy  advertised  wealth  and  "opportunity"  that  lies  only  a  few  miles 
away.  For  them  it  is  as  distant  as  Hawaii.  These  street-wise  young  people, 
who  are  old  by  21,  know  all  the  discrepancies  between  promise  unj 
actuality.  They  know  that  the  promise  of  free  education,  of  job  oppor- 
tunity, of  legal  equality  differs  from  the  actuality  they  experience.  Thcs 
know  that  the  ostensibly  free  clinic  is  a  place  wheie  they  are  rudely 
treated,  and  wheie  a  sick  man  may  suffer  for  many  hours  in  a  waiting  line 
If  they  are  black  or  brown,  they  know  they  may  be  sent  to  jail  while  a 
white  Anglo  is  set  fiee  for  the  identical  offense.  They  may  know  of  jobs, 
yet  in  a  city  like  Los  Angeles  they  cannot  get  to  them  because  they  do  nw 
own  cars  and  theie  is  virtually  no  public  transportation.  Many  of  these 
students  are  blight  and  do  well  in  grammar  school,  but  by  high  school 
they  diop  out  as  they  begin  to  be  inundated  with  financial  and  family 
problems.  If  they  are  spunky  they  are  likely  to  be  pushed  out. 

Traditionally,  the  people  who  have  tried  to  help  have  been  sincere  and 
hardworking  social  workeis,  people  who  still  come  from  comfortable 
middle-class  backgrounds  and  merely  visit  the  ghetto,  escaping  to  aclenn 
quiet  home  each  night.  Despite  their  good  intentions,  social  workers  arc 
inevitably  seen  as  transient  visitois,  and  hated  by  the  people  they  aim  to 
help.  They  are  viewed  as  spies  and  purse  wardens  who  enforce  moialistk 
rules  which  have  no  relevance.  At  an  early  age  many  children  conclude 
that  the  casewoikei  is  merely  snooping  when  he  asks  questions,  ferreting 
out  some  misdemeanor  01  technicality  that  means  the  family  will  noted 
money.  The  familiar  stipulation  that  welfare  recipients  would  receive  no 
money  if  an  able-bodied  man  were  in  the  house  has  created  deceptions 
and  family  disruptions  that  have  warped  the  lives  of  entire  generations. 
The  tragedy  is  typified  by  a  girl  in  the  program  who  fought  and  hated  her 
mother  throughout  her  life,  and  only  later  discoveied  that  her  fathered 
been  driven  from  the  house  by  the  welfare  act,  not  by  her  mother,  By  thai 
time  she  and  her  mothei  were  permanently  estranged. 

Under  pressure  and  without  outlet,  despair,  passivity,  and  withdrawal 
into  multisubstance  use,  such  as  alcohol  and  dangerous  drugs,  are  (lie 
inevitable  concomitants  of  a  life  with  ramified  health  problems,  and 
perpeptual  family  crises.  Hundreds  of  thousands  of  potentially  bright  am! 
creative  youngsters  are  being  warped  and  wasted.  Ghetto  existence  cannot 
be  patched.  Today,  ghetto  youth  live  in  an  ironic  vortex  of  history. 
maiJced  by  the  threat  of  population  growth,  the  exodus  of  middle-class 
families  to  the  subuibs,  inflation,  and  war.  Ironically,  the  pressure  is  on 
the  young,  themselves,  to  rebuild  or  create  the  institutions  that  will 
readmit  the  poor  into  the  mainstieam  of  American  life,  a  life  of  decency 
and  opportunity.  Nobody  else  can  do  the  job. 

Considerable  institutional  and  social  changes  must  be  constructed 
before  this  portion  of  the  American  people  can  participate  in  its  own 
future.  At  present  theie  are  no  institutions  that  can  fully  supply 
city  ghettos  with  the  most  basic  necessities  -  good  housing,  health 
care,  and  education.  The  need  is  for  change  in  institutions,  policies  or 

114 


social  organizations  as  they  now  exist.  In  a  dynamic  society  institutions 
should  change  to  meet  changing  needs.  The  question  that  faces  our  society 
is  how  to  revitalize  our  institutions  so  that  they  maintain  health  and 
well-being.  It  seems  improbable  that  these  institutions  can  be  fashioned  by 
people  who  live  far  from  the  ghettos,  and  who  know  the  problems  only  by 
reading.  The  hope  for  constiuctive  institutional  changes  emanates  from 
within.  Opeiation  Hope  was  evolved  to  select  potential  leaders,  and  give 
these  young  people  the  instiumcnts  for  institutional  change,  supplying 
them  with  the  education  that  is  the  key  to  social  participation.  It  is  hoped 
that  this  program  may  lead  to  changes  in  the  selection  of  peisonnel  for  the 
mental  health  fields. 


History 

Mi.  A.  Paul  Parks,  the  originator  and  guiding  spirit  of  Operation  Hope, 
came  to  Los  Angeles  with  the  conviction  that  his  profession  of  social  work 
needed  to  be  revitalized.  An  Easternei  with  a  varied  work  experience,  he 
had  extensive  experience  in  the  field  of  drug  addiction.  He  had  conducted 
educational  seminars  foi  community  physicians  on  the  social  implications 
of  drug  abuse.  Like  many  of  his  associates,  lie  began  to  see  that  drug 
addiction  and  other  major  social  problems  could  not  be  treated  as  if  they 
were  local  "infections"  that  could  be  diagnosed  and  cuied  on  an  indi- 
vidual basis.  The  traditional  medical  approach  is  rarely  lelevant  in  the 
attempt  to  counter  drug  abuse.  Drugs  of  all  varieties  have  been  the  escape 
of  people  whose  lives  are  intolerable  throughout  the  centuries  and  in  all 
countries  of  the  world.  Life  is  indeed  unendurable  for  the  vciy  poor  and 
no  amount  of  patching  can  alter  the  fundamental  trap  or  the  isolation  in 
which  the  poor,  and  paiticularly  the  black  and  brown  minorities,  now  live. 
Neither  preachment  nor  punishment  can  eradicate  the  alcoholism  and 
drug  use  and  other  consequences  of  a  degrading  mode  of  living  in  a  dis- 
honoring environment. 

The  helping  professions  have  not  always  given  credence  to  the  fact  that 
human  behavior  is  adaptive  and  learned.  This  implies  that  each  commun- 
ity paiticipates  in  shaping  the  lives  of  its  citizens  and  their  behavior.  In 
actuality,  social  work  like  many  other  helping  professions  has  often  used 
its  power  and  position  to  label  behavior  —  an  approach  which  does  not 
produce  the  techniques  that  contribute  to  healthy  reactions  or  change.  In 
gaining  status  as  a  profession,  social  work  in  some  settings  has  also  moved 
further  away  from  the  people  it  originally  set  out  to  work  with. 

In  the  early  1960's,  social  work  groups  were  seeking  new  solutions  to 
ferment  in  the  ghetto,  by  recruiting  and  training  local  young  people  for 
social  work,  Mr.  Parks  moved  West  to  join  an  agency  known  as  Special 
Service  for  Groups,  Inc.,  at  a  lime  when  it  was  training  gang  Icadeis  from 
Watts  and  East  Los  Angeles  as  social  workers'  aides.  He  arrived  in  Los 
Angeles  on  a  hot  August  day  in  1965,  one  day  after  general  discontent 

115 


had  combined  with  heat  of  sumrnei  in  the  destructive  insurrection  that 
came  to  be  known  as  the  Watts  riots. 

Watts  is  a  remnant  of  a  real  estate  tract  in  South  Central  Los  Angeles, 
but  the  action  that  America  remembers  occurred  west  of  Watts  in  an  area 
housing  some  400,000  people,  300,000  of  them  black.  These  people  have 
no  access  to  the  resources  of  the  larger  city.  Pooi,  they  lack  transporta- 
tion, and  are  unable  to  work  outside  their  neighborhood.  Thus,  in  their 
out-of-the-way  enclave,  they  remain  invisible  to  the  affluent  majority. 
Here,  on  a  hot  night  a  small  incident  with  the  police  generated  rumois  and 
crowds  that  slowly  swelled  into  a  populace-police  battle  culminating  in 
the  deaths  of  34  persons  and  the  destruction  of  millions  of  dollars  in 
property.  Watts  became  the  predictive  symbol,  reminding  Americans  that 
poverty  and  racial  oppression  were  hidden  in  the  affluence,  and  thill  the 
problems  of  huge  gioups  of  American  people  were  being  neglected. 'I he 
"Watts  riot"  announced  that  this  double  state  could  not  exist  any  longer. 
Two  years  later  Boyle  Heights  became  the  center  for  the  development  of 
new  solutions,  the  headquarters  for  what  is  now  Operation  Hope. 

Location  and  Staff 

Roughly  two  miles  fiom  the  modern  Civic  Center  of  downtown  Los 
Angeles  is  an  old  barrio  -  Boyle  Heights.  With  the  adjacent  neighborhoods 
of  East  Los  Angeles  and  City  Terrace,  it  houses  about  180,000  people  of 
whom  135,000  have  Spanish  surnames.  Boyle  Heights  is  bounded  by  free- 
ways, an  area  of  150  blocks  through  which  many  waves  of  immignilioii 
have  passed.  Like  a  port  of  entry  to  the  city,  it  has  received  successive 
waves  of  Russians,  Serbians,  Italians,  Jews,  Mexicans,  and  othcis.  At 
present  Spanish  is  the  major  language  and  many  of  the  older  Mexican- 
Americans  carry  on  the  traditions  of  their  mother  country.  In  the  ghettos 
and  barrios  of  South,  Central,  and  East  Los  Angeles  few  new  homes  have 
been  built  for  20  years,  but  as  middle-income  people  fled  to  the  suburbs, 
black  and  brown  people  took  their  place.  It  is  in  the  center  of  this  inulU- 
lingual  ghetto  that  Operation  Hope  was  established  in  1968,  setting  up 
offices  in  a  few  rented  rooms. 

The  four  staff  offices,  the  large  anteroom  and  classroom  were  carefully 
chosen,  for  these  are  not  only  the  working  headquarters  of  Operation 
Hope,  but  the  center  of  all  activities,  seminars,  and  consultations,  the  one 
meeting  ground  for  its  many  disparate  participants.  The  rooms  arc 
spacious  and  friendly,  containing  the  bulletin  boards  of  events,  duplicating 
equipment,  and  an  incipient  reference  library  for  students, 

Mr.  Parks  drew  together  a  staff  as  varied  in  completion  and  background 
as  the  students  he  would  seek  out  It  had  to  be  a  group  of  exception- 
ally experienced  and  committed  people,  because  the  entire  staff  would 
include  only  five  people,  and  they  could  anticipate  working  around  the 
clock,  in  every  conceivable  exigency.  The  staff  all  had  extensive  social 
work  experience,  and  each  person  came  from  a  different  part  of  the 
country  as  well  as  different  ethnic  backgrounds,  They  are  supplemented 
by  consultants  and  instructors. 

116 


In  philosophy  the  staff  agreed.  They  had  all  personally  seen  the  failuie 
of  the  medical  model  of  social  work.  They  had,  themselves,  been  trained 
to  act  as  professional  social  physicians  who  would  diagnose  a  needy 
client's  problems  and  try  to  repair  them  in  the  manner  of  setting  a  broken 
bone.  Since  the  ills  almost  inevitably  lie  in  the  complex  social  environ- 
ment, not  the  individual,  the  medical  approach  to  social  work  is  extremely 
frustrating.  The  staff,  therefore,  felt  that  a  new  approach  was  essential 
that  the  people  should  be  selected  for  the  helping  professions,  out  of 
ghetto  areas,  not  the  middle  class,  with  the  hope  that  these  indigenous 
people  would  invoke  needed  social  and  institutional  changes.  Foitunately, 
one  member  of  the  staff  has  long  been  a  resident  in  the  neighboihood,  and 
is  known  among  many  people  in  Boyle  Heights.  It  is  rare  for  a  project  to 
have  on  tap  a  person  who  is  familiar  with  the  history  and  problems  of  the 
people  just  outside  the  door. 

Many  Los  Angeles  people  from  wealthy  neighborhoods  have  visited 
Mexico,  but  have  not  heard  of  Boyle  Heights  only  a  few  miles  away.  They 
would  feel  as  out  of  place  there,  as  in  a  foreign  country.  It  is  a  distance 
and  contrast  that  cannot  be  overemphasized,  for  Los  Angeles  sprawls  over 
an  area  of  about  400  square  miles.  Among  its  eight  million  people,  some 
epitomize  the  wealthiest  in  suburban  living,  while  close  to  a  million  repre- 
sent the  most  hopeless  isolation  of  poverty.  The  facade  of  poverty  in  Los 
Angeles  is  prettier  than  that  of  Eastern  cities  because  there  are  houses  and 
trees,  but  the  isolation  is  more  exaggerated  because  people  who  do  not  own 
cars  cannot  leave  then  neighborhoods.  Beaches,  museums,  plays,  and  con- 
certs, taken  for  granted  by  middle-class  youth,  cannot  be  reached  without 
some  form  of  transportation.  It  is  worth  repeating  that  jobs  and  schools 
are  similarly  out  of  reach  for  those  who  cannot  afford  cars.  Thus,  the 
misleading  facade  of  small  white  houses  and  palm  trees  belies  a  greater 
isolation  and  deeper  abandonment  than  the  visual  filth  of  a  New  York 
City  "slum." 

Most  of  the  institutions  that  might  change  the  viscious  cycle  of  mental 
illness  and  misery  that  takes  place  in  the  ghetto  are  also  too  far  away.  The 
people  who  make  the  decisions  allocating  money  and  setting  up  basic 
requirements  for  housing,  health,  and  education  are  far  removed  from  the 
realities  of  the  poor,  particularly  the  poor  minorities,  the  blacks,  Chicanos 
(the  current  local  term  for  socially  active  Mexican-Americans)  of  the  West 
and  Indians  throughout  the  country.  Middle-income  Americans  hear  sta- 
tistics about  Government  budgets  and  visualize  free,  tax-supported  educa- 
tion that  is  available  to  all,  as  well  as  free  clinics  in  hospitals,  that  make 
medical  care  accessible  to  everyone.  The  services  are  available  on  paper.  If 
they  are  to  be  made  a  reality  for  the  people  who  need  them,  it  will  happen 
when  ghetto  people  themselves,  have  the  education  and  credentials  to 
attain  positions  of  authority.  As  all  the  great  voices  from  the  depths  of 
ghetto  life  have  tried  to  explain,  the  forces  of  jungle  survival  bruise  and 
also  strengthen  a  person,  thus  creating  an  individual  for  whom  the  games 
of  middle-class  education  are  particularly  difficult.  The  very  people  who 
must  lead  ghetto  improvements  are  therefore  not  easy  to  mould;  they 
have  needs  that  are  not  easily  met. 

117 


Recruiting  the  Model  Trainee 

In  discussions  with  a  community  psychiatrist,  a  psychologist,  and  social 
workers,  Mr.  Parks  and  his  staff  had  evolved  a  profile  of  desnable  traits  -a 
checklist  that  might  be  used  in  inteiviewing  prospective  trainees.  They 
wanted  youngsters  who  were  dissatisfied,  but  not  passively  disgi  untied, 
students  who  wished  to  bring  about  change,  yet  who  had  attiactive  per- 
sonalities They  needed  young  people  with  fire  who  wanted  to  learn  rather 
than  destroy;  people  with  a  sense  of  justice  and  motivation.  How  did  one 
find  a  young  man  in  the  ghetto  with  fire  and  talent  who  had  not  been  loo 
embittered,  beaten,  legally  entangled,  mentally  warped,  or  addicted  lo 
drugs?  The  young  people  had  to  have  developed  the  strength  to  survive  in 
the  ghetto.  They  had  to  be  people  who  knew  how  to  fight,  were  wise  to 
every  con,  and  canny  in  the  brutal  realities  of  their  own  neighborhoods. 
At  the  same  time,  they  were  going  to  be  asked  to  incorporate  a  new  style 
of  living,  adopt  a  genteel  mannei,  and  survive  in  another  world  of  middle- 
class  college  life,  learning  the  subtleties  of  bureaucracy,  and  the  language 
of  the  affluent  woild.  They  had  to  be  people  who  could  survive  1.1 
both  life  styles  simultaneously.  These  qualities  should  strike  the  leader  as 
remarkable,  given  the  environment  from  which  the  trainees  were  sought 
Ft  is  astonishing  that  people  with  such  qualifications  were  actually  found, 
Most  of  them  had  been  in  trouble  with  the  law.  Some  had  been  involved 
with  drugs.  Any  youngster  with  the  spirit  and  independence  of  mind  to 
become  a  leader  was  automatically  too  spirited  to  be  considered  desirable 
by  teachers  and  educational  authorities.  Yet,  it  was  precisely  the  school 
"troublemakers,"  the  questioners,  fighters,  activists,  who  were  the  objects 
of  the  search. 

The  Opeiation  Hope  staff  began  their  quest  by  asking  for  refenals  fiom 
social  workers  and  teachers.  After  the  Watts  riot,  Central  Los  Angeles  was 
inundated  with  community  programs.  Nonetheless,  when  the  staff  looked 
for  applicants  from  the  Educational  Clearinghouses,  from  schools,  social 
agencies,  or  ethnic  organizations,  these  agencies  could  not  pioducc  a  single 
person  to  fit  the  criteria.  In  general,  schools  and  agencies  were  antago- 
nistic to  the  aggiessiveness  and  spirit  that  would  qualify  a  student,  and  in 
the  end  they  did  not  help  at  all. 

Not  many  social  workers  or  teachers  actually  live  in  Boyle  Heights  or 
other  ghetto  areas.  The  ghetto's  daily  rhythm  begins  with  an  immigration 
and  ends  with  an  exodus.  From  the  arrival  of  milkmen  in  the  morning,  the 
police,  the  teachers,  storekeepers,  bankers,  and  social  workers  flow  into 
the  area.  In  mid-afternoon,  when  the  teachers  begin  driving  to  their  homes 
20  and  30  miles  away,  the  exodus  begins.  In  this  ebb  and  flow,  the  teacher 
or  social  worker  is,  in  fact,  a  stranger,  a  visitor,  who  does  not  know  the 
families  of  the  children  he  teaches  or  people  he  tries  to  help. 

After  six  months  of  searching  the  network  of  social  agencies  and  the 
neighborhoods,  the  staff  of  Operation  Hope  had  to  change  its  initial  image 
of  a  potential  leader  and  the  means  of  finding  him.  Many  of  the  young 
people  with  leadership  qualities  had  been  so  emotionally  damaged  and 
brutalized  during  childhood  that  they  could  not  remain  sensitive  to  other 

118 


people,  Often  they  were  withdrawn  and  suspicious.  The  staff  began  to 
look  at  the  habits  of  potential  candidates,  seeking  people  who  had  spent 
time  helping  others.  Ultimately  they  did  find  anomalous  people,  a  man 
from  Watts  who  was  teaching  youngsters  by  coaching  them  in  -sports,  u  girl 
who  had  worked  with  Head  'Start.  After  interviewing  some  60  people,  fom 
prospective  students  were  found,  and  through  them  a  gtiivepine  was  begun 
that  finally  attracted  some  200  others.  The  kinds  of  students  who  had 
unbroken  spirits,  who  wished  to  help  others,  and  could  use  their  anger  at 
social  injustice  for  constructive  change  were  to  be  found  on  street  corners, 
in  picket  lines  or  community  demonstrations,  not  in  welfaie  agencies. 

After  months  of  recruiting,  the  staff  found  33  qualified  applicants,  bill 
they  had  funds  for  only  20.  They  selected  the  people  who  seemed  to  have 
the  rarest  combination  of  qualities,  leadership  ability,  and  sensitivity  lo 
other  people.  Now  they  were  asking  these  students  to  hold  their  auger  and 
their  action  in  abeyance,  to  take  on  a  middle-class  life  style,  and  to  posl- 
pone  their  effective  action  until  -  perhaps  seven  or  eight  ycms  later-  (hey 
had  their  credentials.  It  meant  a  long-term  commitment  to  mi  educational 
program,  postponing  rewards,  and  suppressing  many  of  their  normal  feel- 
ings. At  a  time  when  many  of  the  disgruntled  youth  of  the  United  States 
had  given  up  hope  of  working  through  institutions,  these  students  were 
asked  to  gamble  on  the  hope  that  they  could  later  constructively  change 
conditions  through  the  instruments  of  the  establishment.  This  is  some 
measure  of  the  remarkable  men  and  women  who  have  been  chosen  to 
participate.  To  them,  indeed,  the  program  seemed  unreal  at  (list.  As  one 
man  said, 

"At  fust,  I  thought  you  people  were  from  Mars,  then  I  decided 
you  were  the  'fuzz'  trying  to  find  out  what  minority  people  were 
doing.  Right  now  I  do  not  know  what  your  bag  is  because  I  know 
nobody  gives  a  person  money  to  go  to  school,  but  it's  a  groovy 
program,  and  I  am  making  B's  for  the  first  time  in  my  life." 

The  Col  leges 

Originally,  the  staff  had  planned  to  pick  20  trainees  and  supply  them 
with  money  enough  to  graduate  from  a  State  or  city  college,  while  offer- 
ing them  special  courses  in  social  sciences  and  remedial  work.  It  was  soon 
apparent  that  the  tax-supported  schools  were  not  amenable  to  these  stu- 
dents despite  their  facilities.  Theoretically,  California  offers  virtually  free 
education  for  all  qualified  residents.  Actually,  the  State  colleges  charge 
about  $150  a  year  and  some  fees  are  over  $200,  an  amount  that  is  formid- 
able to  these  students.  In  addition,  books  cost  as  much  as  $125  a  year 
State  universities  charge  $775  per  year  plus  special  fees  and  books. 

Most  of  the  ghetto  youngsters  have  missed  classes  In  order  to  work  or 
have  dropped  put  of  high  school  in  discouragement;  one  girl  dropped  oui 
of  high  school  two  weeks  before  graduation,  and  nobody  in  her  school 
acted  to  get  her  a  diploma.  Many  others  have  been  flunked  out  for  ques- 
tioning the  teacher  or  expressing  opinions.  Since  the  State  universities 


admit  only  from  those  students  in  the  top  12  percent  of  the  high  school 
giaduates,  while  the  State  colleges  accept  students  from  the  top  Nurd, 
most  ghetto  students  could  not  qualify,  however  innately  intelligent  they 
were.  State  colleges  may  admit  2  percent  of  incoming  classes  as  members 
of  minority  groups  and  another  2  percent  who  show  promise  despite  low 
test  scores.  On  the  other  hand  community  colleges  take  anyone  with  a 
high  school  diploma.  A  few  of  the  trainees  had  already  entered  local 
two-year  colleges  on  theii  own,  but  the  administrative  bureaucracy  was  so 
frustiating  that  after  two-and-a-half  years  one  student  had  never  hncl  tfie 
opportunity  to  discuss  his  curriculum  with  an  advisor.  He  had  taken  end- 
less courses  and  yet  had   not  fulfilled   the  requirements  to  giiidtciti:. 
Another  student  had  piled  up  30  course  credits  without  completing  any  of 
the  basic  couises  to  graduate.  At  the  largei  schools,  the  State  colleges,  Hie 
coldness  and  rigidity  of  the  administiation  made  the  students  fed  they 
were  not  wanted.  The  unfamiliar  routine  of  application  forms  and  inter- 
views lesemble  social  welfare  agencies.  As  one  student  had  said  about 
bureaucracy,  "The  place  is  designed  to  frustrate  you.  By  the  time  you 
explain  what  your  problem  is  to  a  number  of  people,  answer  endless 
questions  regarding  why  you  had  the  problem,  fill  out  endless  papers  -iind 
the  waiting  is  awful  -  you  will  be  told  you  will  be  seen  at  such  and  suit 
time  by  Mr.  so  and  so,  and  you  wait  some  more." 

It  was  finally  necessary  for  the  Operation  Hope  staff  to  negotiate 
scholarships  for  their  trainees  at  small  private  institutions,  and  the  s(n- 
dents  were  finally  placed  at  Occidental  College,  Pepperdinc,  Wittier, 
Marymount,  USC,  and  other  schools  around  the  metropolitan  area.  Tlic 
students  had  to  be  helped  through  high  school  equivalency  exams,  the 
red-tape  of  application  foims  and  transcripts,  and  enrollment.  They 
needed  additional  funds  for  books,  and  continuous  emotional  support, 
One  college  administiative  official  told  Operation  Hope  staff:  "We  will 
consider  applicants  from  your  program  as  long  as  you  are  available  to  give 
support.  From  our  experience  minority  students  will  not  use  school 
counselors  within  the  institution." 

The  process  of  educating  young  men  and  women  may  seem  a  straight- 
forward matter  of  funding  and  organization  but  it  is  a  subtle  and  ramified 
undertaking.  Unless  helped  with  all  the  exigencies  of  survival,  with  Jiving 
needs,  books,  and  emergencies,  the  student  would  have  to  be  absorbed  by 
the  all-demanding  hustle  to  exist.  Although  studying  may  ultimately  liber- 
ate the  student,  the  first  impact  is  one  of  extreme  tension.  Students  liavt 
to  survive  not  only  at  home,  but  in  a  new  environment  that  is  hostile,  on  a 
campus  whose  very  philosophy  is  antagonistic  to  them.  For  instance, 
Chicane  students  are  affronted  to  find  that  their  hero,  Cesar  Chavez,  is 
considered  a  communist  agitator  by  many  students,  and  the  black  stu- 
dents are  amazed  to  learn  that  the  charismatic  Malcolm  X  is  considered 
merely  a  criminal  by  middle-class  whites.  This  is  tantamount  to  sending  a 
devout  Christian  among  people  who  call  Jesus  a  communist  provocateur. 
On  campus  the  values,  dress,  language,  and  social  life  are  all  unfamiliar  and 
somewhat  threatening.  Moreover,  the  average  college  student  is  still  partly 
a  child,  involved  and  dependent  upon  his  family,  while  the  ghetto  student 

120 


has  been  a  street-wise  adult  for  some  years,  depending  upon  his  wits,  often 
supporting  or  helping  to  support  a  family.  For  the  white  middle-class 
student,  college  may  be  a  slight  strain,  a  first  venture  away  from  the 
comfortable  family  enclaves.  For  the  ghetto  student  it  is  another  country, 
one  in  which  he  is  scorned  because  he  does  not  know  the  school  game,  a 
woild  in  which  he  feels  he  hostility  and  cruelty  of  people  who  have  been 
comfortable  all  their  lives. 

One  gifted  student  in  Operation  Hope  commented  that  money  to  facil- 
itate college  was  the  most  important  contiibution  any  project  could  have 
made  to  his  life.  No  other  Federal  piogram  operates  precisely  this  way. 
Still,  in  attempting  to  help  these  students  through  college,  theOpeiation 
Hope  experience  emphasizes  how  wide  is  the  breach  between  rich  and 
poor  in  America  today. 

Operation  Hope 

The  staff  and  directors'  offices  at  Operation  Hope  are  open  at  all  times 
for  students,  and  a  "hot  line"  telephone  is  maintained  at  night  and  on 
weekends.  At  least  one  staff  member  is  always  available  to  help  with 
psychiatric  emergencies,  health  problems,  draft  exigencies,  or  legal  pro- 
blems. If  a  staff  member  vanishes  for  several  hours,  or  remains  at  his  office 
until  late  at  night,  it  usually  means  that  a  student  is  in  a  crisis,  These 
students  acquired  adult  problems  before  they  entered  college.  One 
21-year-old,  for  example,  needed  medical  and  legal  counseling  concerning 
divorce  and  the  death  of  his  2-year-old  child.  Draft  counseling  is  another 
important  need.  Since  most  of  the  men  have  jobs  and  families,  they  can- 
not finance  the  30  hours  of  couise  time  that  would  qualify  them  for 
student  deferments.  Thus,  consultations  and  appeals  to  local  draft  boards 
are  necessary  staff  functions.  These  students  also  need  advice  about 
courses,  professors,  and  examinations.  They  require  coaching  in  the  tech- 
niques of  studying,  paper  writing,  and  exam  taking.  All  of  these  areas  are 
routinely  part  of  staff  guidance. 

These  students  had  been  so  intellectually  and  emotionally  starved  that 
they  devoured  attention.  None  of  them  had  enjoyed  the  kinds  of  family 
or  social  contacts  that  would  develop  intellectual  curiosity.  Indeed,  most 
of  them  had  such  battered  images  of  themselves  they  were  afraid  to  ask 
questions  in  college  classes.  Many  of  them  needed  someone  with  whom 
they  could  discuss  the  Viet  Nam  War,  the  tax  system,  the  meaning  of  the 
moon  shot,  or  racial  discrimination.  One  student  who  began  the  program 
with  a  poor  self-image,  put  it  this  way:  "Now  I  can  question,  but  I  lack 
power,  I've  learned  how  to  conform  enough  to  get  by  ...  ."  The  staff, 
who  are  black,  white,  and  Chicano,  are  by  turns  the  surrogate  parents, 
doctors,  lawyers,  philosophers,  and  companions  to  whom  the  students  can 
turn. 

Since  students  live  in  perpetual  crisis,  staff  members  know  there  will 
never  be  a  weekend  without  its  emergency,  no  night  without  a  call  from  a 
panicked  student.  The  services  of  Operation  Hope  span  the  gamut  from 

121 


counseling  and  financial  assistance  to  aid  in  getting  things  done.  This  may 
mean  calling  a  medical  doctor,  getting  a  family  welfare  check  expedited, 
or  getting  a  family  member  admitted  into  a  hospital.  Any  middle-class 
person  takes  for  granted  that  he  can  lift  a  telephone  to  accomplish  such 
things,  but  these  needs  can  become  major  crises  for  the  ghetto  person.  The 
students  needed  these  reality  services,  as  well  as  step-by-step  advice  on 
how  to  dress,  how  to  behave  at  an  interview,  and  lessons  in  the  social 
amenities,  the  things  white  students  take  for  granted.  They  needed  lessons 
in  speech,  training  in  group  speaking.  Many  of  them  needed  to  learn  how 
to  write,  to  organize  papers  and  express  their  ideas  cleaily.  Because  the 
campus  atmosphere  is  cold  and  unfamiliar,  the  one  comfortable  phice 
where  all  the  participants  could  meet  has  been  at  classes  offered  by  Opera- 
tion Hope. 

Campus  lectures,  particularly  in  the  social  sciences,  have  frustrated  and 
angered  these  students.  Often  the  courses  seemed  inelevant  or  instructors 
offered  opinions  that  sounded  incredibly  naive  to  the  ears  of  these  ex- 
perienced, old-young  men  and  women. 

One  of  the  important  needs  of  the  students  was  a  sense  of  ethnic 
identification  and  history,  and  a  forum  where  they  could  freely  raise 
questions  about  universal  social  ideas.  Courses  in  black  or  Chicano  history 
are  often  badly  taught  in  colleges  Indeed,  these  courses  often  are  taught 
with  a  condescending  point  of  view  that  insults  the  student  who  is 
attempting  to  learn  about  his  own  heritage.  Consequently,  such  courses 
have  been  taught  at  the  Operation  Hope  headquarters.  During  the  fall 
semester  of  1 970,  June  Moore  of  the  UCLA  School  of  Social  Work,  taught 
a  course  on  the  history  and  philosophy  of  social  work  institutions  in  the 
United  States.  The  informality  of  such  classes  permits  the  students  lo 
interact  with  each  other  and  with  teachers  in  informal  discussion  and 
comradery  that  is  lacking  in  most  of  their  lives. 

Involuntarily  most  of  these  students  are  loners.  During  the  seven-eiglil 
years  when  they  are  holding  jobs  and  also  studying,  they  are  under  un- 
usual strain.  They  need  the  staff  help  in  homemaking,  in  finding  an  apart- 
ment, buying  a  car  or  filing  insurance.  Many  of  them  have  debts,  often 
from  the  illegal  claims  of  door-to-door  salesmen. 

The  small  "Hope"  offices,  with  their  modest  library  and  open  doors, 
provide  a  kind  of  home  base  for  these  students  where  they  can  "rap"  with 
the  staff,  and  admit  their  real  feelings,  or  ask  for  help.  It  is  the  only  such 
place  in  their  lives.  Not  at  home,  at  friends'  houses,  nor  in  school  or  clinics 
can  they  find  support  and  assistance. 

By  contrast  with  the  impersonal,  bureaucratic  schools,  and  institutions, 

the  staff  has  an  open-door  policy  which  means  nobody  is  ever  too  busy  to 

HiHpnt  wnen  he  drops  in.  There  is  never  a  wait.  Since  the  students 

r  *he  program,  staff  members  are  always  on  hand  to  help 

+o  provide  coffee  and  a  snack.  Students  rely  on  tin's 

jring  exam  periods,  or  when  a  term  paper  is  due. 
wvmg  db  mey  do,  in  two  very  separate  life  styles,  the  students  are 
under  extreme  tension.  As  one  Chicano  put  it,  "This  project  is  great  but, 
man,  am  I  having  problems.  For  the  first  time  I  am  learning  to  think,  I 

122 


listen  and  try  to  figure  out  what's  being  said.  I'm  so  excited  about  hearing 
people,  but  I'm  bored  with  my  family.  I'm  finding  my  girlfiiend  a  drag, 
we  can't  talk  anymore.  And  the  guys  I  know,  well  I  haven't  seen  them  in 
weeks.  Man,  is  this  lonely." 

For  ghetto  students  in  the  middle-class  colleges  the  strain  of  a  double 
life  is  continuous.  Theie  is  an  internal  price  they  pay  foi  living  in  two 
worlds.  Part  of  the  price  may  be  in  terms  of  physical  tensions,  anxieties, 
or  general  loss  of  self-confidence.  Realistically,  life  is  a  daily  struggle.  In 
Older  to  study,  the  ghetto  student  leads  a  tired  and  lonely  life.  As  one 
man  from  Watts  described  his  schedule,  he  attends  school  in  the  morning, 
works  afternoons,  sleeps  in  the  early  evenings,  and  studies  all  night,  after 
his  family  goes  to  bed.  He  has  no  time  or  energy  for  close  peisonal 
relationships  The  stress  is  such  that  he  must  keep  his  feelings  undci 
control  at  all  times.  It  is  not  surprising  that  he  lias  a  stomach  ulcer.  Many 
of  the  students  in  the  project  suffer  from  psychosomatic  ailments  al- 
though they  are  very  young.  Some  have  hives,  or  colitis,  ulcers,  or  hyper- 
tension. A  few  suffer  from  insomnia,  panic,  and  nightmares.  When  a  stu- 
dent leaves  the  personal  and  familiar  world  of  the  barrio  for  the  college 
environment,  he  leaves  his  status  and  sense  of  acceptance,  and  must  adapt 
to  a  highly  impersonal  structure.  He  must  be  careful  in  his  dress,  guarded 
in  his  manner  and  speech.  When  he  leaves  campus  he  again  returns  to  a 
wife  (01  a  woman  may  leturn  to  a  husband)  whose  standards  and  outlook 
are  the  antithesis  of  all  that  the  campus  represents. 

To  the  average  student  on  campus,  the  ghetto  student  seems  of  a  dif- 
ferent breed.  Tough,  street-wise,  analytical,  many  of  them  have  experi- 
enced jails,  and  brutalities  that  the  other  students  have  only  lead  about. 
They  have  suffered  hunger,  exhaustion,  and  have  seen  their  friends  turn  to 
alcohol  or  drugs.  They  have  had  to  use  cunning  and  muscle  to  survive.  An 
affluent  youngster  arrives  in  college  certain  that  theft  and  alcoholism  arc 
signs  of  weakness,  inferiority.  Neither  students  nor  their  teachcis  realize 
that  the  crimes  they  deplore  may  be  tlie  vehicles  of  survival  in  the  ghetto. 
The  kid  who  doesn't  suivivc  that  jungle  docs  not  get  to  enter  a  project 
that  will  help  him  through  college.  A  person  who  has  not  used  his  cunning 
to  suivive  will  ultimately  despair,  entering  the  no-mans-lancl  of  inertia  and 
drugs.  A  ghetto  student  who  seems  angry,  suppressed,  cynical,  and  hard 
can  only  be  understood  if  his  armor  of  hostility  and  suspicion  are  also 
understood;  young  people  who  have  lived  in  the  ghetto  or  barrio  foi  20 
years  have  developed  defenses  that  allow  them  to  cope  with  drug  users, 
police  harrassment,  with  rejections  by  schools,  social  agencies  and  hos- 
pitals. Most  of  them  have  needed  to  devise  a  hard  surface,  never  revealing 
their  feelings  to  anyone,  never  trusting  anyone,  always  analyzing  a  situa- 
tion for  possible  traps.  Since  they  have  had  to  hustle  for  money  all  then 
lives,  they  may  be  busy  trying  to  beat  the  welfaie  system,  or  avoiding 
someone  else's  con  game.  Typically,  as  children,  some  of  the  students  have 
been  shifted  from  one  foster  home  to  another,  in  the  manner  of  one  girl 
whose  mother  was  declared  mentally  incompetent.  At  age  5  she  recalled 
being  put  to  bed  in  a  relative's  house  where,  as  she  hugged  the  pillow,  she 
found  a  gun.  For  her,  school  was  &  relief  from  washing,  cooking,  cleaning, 

123 


and  being  beaten  with  a  belt  buckle  arbitrarily.  At  14,  she  and  a  friend 
weie  put  in  jail  for  stealing  Anothci  girl  was  abandoned  at  the  age  of  14 
and  left  alone  in  an  apartment.  In  ordei  not  to  be  evicted  she  rented 
looms,  but  when  she  tued  to  go  on  welfare  to  raise  the  rent  money,  the 
casework ei  told  hei  to  thiow  a  rent  patty  and  charge  admission  which 
could  be  used  to  pay  the  month's  rent-  Two  weeks  before  high  school 
graduation  the  girl  quit  school  and  worked  at  odd  jobs  foi  six  years  before 
anyone  suggested  that  she  had  a  good  mind  and  might  go  to  college.  To 
the  son  of  a  migiant  worker,  an  avid  readei  in  a  large  family,  the  possibil- 
ity of  a  college  education  seemed  like  the  wildest  of  dreams  as  he  worked 
in  the  fields  year  after  yeat ,  missing  months  of  school. 

To  classmates  or  outsiders  these  students  might  seem  cool  and  brusque, 
with  an  appearance  of  self-confidence.  Actually,  they  of  ten  live  in  a  stole 
of  conflict  that  is  bordering  on  panic.  They  know  that  they  must  be  many 
times  as  strong,  intelligent,  and  controlled  as  their  peers  in  school,  and 
that  they  are  expected  to  endure  and  prevail  in  a  manner  that  would  be 
impossible  for  many  adults,  Raised  by  television,  these  students  havea 
sophistication  that  is  underestimated  by  their  peeis.  They  know  a  good 
deal  about  the  way  they  look  to  the  other  side,  and  they  are  familial  with 
the  comforts  of  most  Americans,  with  the  social  work  jargon  in  which 
ghetto  disadvantages  are  discussed,  and  they  realize  that  they  arc  put 
down  for  qualities  that  are  really  the  product  of  their  environment,  They 
must  survive  in  two  worlds,  and  ate  expected  to  be  idealistic  when  they 
are  surrounded  by  despaii  and  futility  and  to  be  willing  to  give  some  part 
of  their  lives  to  change  a  society  that  would  not  ordinarily  give  them  a 
chance. 

By  adolescence  most  ghetto  children  have  given  up.  They  see  no  alter- 
natives and  no  future.  In  the  southern  section  of  Central  Los  Angeles  they 
are  likely  to  turn  to  alcohol,  while  the  eastern  section  they  would  more 
likely  use  drugs.  Many  youngsters  between  15  and  25  try  or  actually  do 
commit  suicide.  Theie  is  nobody  to  help  them.  Their  families  are  dis- 
rupted. Teachers  who  have  taught  in  these  neighborhoods  for  20  years, 
but  who  live  elsewhere,  do  not  even  know  the  parents  of  their  students. 
As  one  black  student  explained,  high  school  students  are  never  told  thai 
they  have  possibilities  of  further  education.  Typically,  one  student  stated, 
"Nobody  evci  told  me  1  was  college  material."  One  very  able  athlete,  wilh 
honor  roll  marks,  wasted  two  years  after  high  school  not  knowing  that  he 
could  have  attended  a  tax-supported  college  without  paying  thousands  of 
dollars.  Scholarship  offers  had  been  made  through  his  high  school  because 
of  his  athletic  ability,  but  he  had  never  been  informed  by  his  advisors. 
Another  girl  wasted  six  years  in  dead-end  jobs  because  she  similarly  did 
not  know  college  was  even  a  remote  possibility.  Isolated  from  the  infor- 
mation that  every  middle-income  person  takes  for  granted,  and  misled  by 
their  own  schools,  the  best  of  these  youngsters  have  a  bitter  tale  to  tell. 
By  age  24  many  of  them  say  they  feel  50,  On  a  college  campus  this  is  no 
advantage. 

124 


Impact 

The  first  most  important  impact  of  Operation  Hope  was  the  fact  that 
the  students  were  sticking  out  their  difficult  academic  programs.  Pre- 
viously, when  faced  with  crises  they  had  withdrawn  from  college.  In  this 
project,  bolstered  by  the  staff,  they  weatheied  continual  crises  and  man- 
aged to  go  on  studying.  Even  success  was  not  without  conflict.  One  girl 
who  had,  with  considerable  help  made  a  C  in  English,  remarked,  "Well,  I 
guess  this  means  I've  given  up.  I'm  conforming,  the  fight  is  over,  I've 
entered  the  system  -  what  can  I  possibly  do  to  make  anything  better  for 
myself  or  anybody  else.  I  did  what  the  instiuctoi  wanted  and  I  passed,  he 
wasn't  the  least  interested  in  what  I  am,  what  I  feel,  or  what  I  want" 

The  students'  marks  have  steadily  improved  and  several  begun  to  be 
eloquent  spokesmen,  writing  reasoned  essays  on  the  issues  close  to  theii 
hearts.  Drug  abuse  is  a  topic  on  which  these  students  have  more  than  usual 
understanding  while  the  illegality  of  the  drugs  generates  the  crime  for 
which  they  aie  punished,  it  seems  clear  to  them  that  there  is  little  concern 
foi  actually  helping  addicts;  there  aie  no  adequate  medical  or  community- 
based  programs  available.  One  of  the  students  wiote: 

"How  rational  would  it  be  to  appoint  a  doctor  as  a  judge  in  a 
court  of  law?  It  would  seem  that  this  wouldn't  be  very  rational  at  all, 
for  although  he  might  know  medicine  he  would  be  quite  unfamiliar 
with  the  mechanics  of  law.  Taken  in  this  light  the  question  is  laised: 
Why  are  administratois  and  law  enforcement  officials  considered  to 
be  more  knowledgeable  in  the  aiea  of  a  medical  and  social  pioblem 
such  as  drug  addiction?" 

The  participants  are  impressive  people.  Selected  for  qualities  of  leader- 
ship, they  also  have  displayed  considerable  aesthetic  talents.  Many  of 
them  write  excellent  poetry,  sing,  and  paint.  Candid  and  cynical,  they  are 
at  the  same  time  deep  and  more  idealistic.  Although  the  men  and  women 
supported  by  the  project  have  come  from  different  homes,  their  stories 
are  painfully  similar.  Most  of  them  come  from  families  on  relief.  In  grade 
school  they  were  demeaned  by  their  teachers.  They  weie  often  absent 
from  school,  and  were  arrested  at  an  early  age  for  some  theft  such  as 
gouging  nickles  from  a  parking  meter  or  for  gang  fighting,  By  17  the 
"average"  young  man  had  been  in  a  house  of  correction,  may  have  gotten 
a  girlfriend  pregnant  and  have  had  to  marry,  spending  a  year  or  so  washing 
cars  or  doing  clean-up  work  in  a  factory.  By  age  20,  they  could  see 
themselves  at  a  dead  end,  with  the  odds  stacked  against  them.  Yet  they 
had  intense  intellectual  curiosity,  and  some  inner  stature  that  was  not 
altered  by  the  meanness  of  life  around  them.  As  one  girl  remarked: 

"A  long  time  ago  I  made  up  my  mind  I  would  rather  die  than  to 
treat  people  as  I  had  been  treated,  I  knew  what  it  meant  to  be 
disliked  for  nothing  and  always  used,  I  suppose  having  compassion 
has  been  the  only  thing  that  has  saved  me  from  hating  the  world." 

One  Chicano  student  recalls  being  "busted"  on  a  marijuana  charge. 
When  he  appeared  in  court  he  was  sent  to  jail,  while  a  white  kid  on  the 
identical  charge  was  released,  "Jail  is  a  school  where  you  learn  many 

125 


things,"  His  experience  in  the  army  taught  him  yet  more  about  discrimina- 
tion as  he  watched  some  episodes  of  mistreatment  of  the  Vietnamese. 
Now,  on  his  leturn,  he  had  a  mission,  to  create  changes,  and  as  a  beginning 
he  has  been  tutoring  kids  in  an  East  Los  Angeles  Paiole  Centei.  His  own 
image  of  his  past  life  was  succinctly  expressed  in  a  newspaper  essay  he 
wrote: 

"I  cannot  help  but  bring  up  a  thought  that  would  enter  my  mind 
every  once  in  a  while  in  jail.  I  would  picture  myself  as  a  peison  trying 
to  leain  how  to  swim  (cope  with  every  clay  life  problems)  in  a  deep, 
dark,  cold  sea  (the  Barrio),  and  finding  myself  submerging  because  of 
my  inability  to  swim.  Along  comes  a  lifeguard  (institution:  rehabili- 
tation) and  pulls  me  up  on  the  platform  (institution)  and  shows  me 
how  to  swim  (rehabilitates  me)  and  simply  throws  me  back  into  the 
deep,  dark,  cold  sea  (Barrio)  with  little  knowledge  as  to  whether  I 
had  developed  the  ability  to  swim.  .  . ." 

Although  many  of  the  students  in  the  program  came  from  disrupted  or 
nonexistent  families,  a  few  have  shown  the  stability  that  comes  from  tacit 
family  support.  A  student  from  Watts,  who  has  watched  his  friends  de- 
teriorate along  the  route  of  drugs  and  apathy,  somehow  manages  lo 
study  while  his  family  sleeps.  He  is  lucky  enough  to  have  a  family  thai  is 
together.  He  also  manages  to  continue  athletics,  coaching  younger  chil- 
dren in  school  subjects  and  basketball.  He  manages  to  live  without  close 
friends,  without  a  confidante  or  wife,  without  time  for  himself.  He  works 
in  the  afternoon,  sleeps  until  midnight,  studies  until  dawn,  and  goes  to 
school  in  the  morning.  When  he  finally  has  his  credentials  he  wants  to 
become  a  poverty  lawyer.  He  has  every  reason  a  man  could  have  to  play 
an  escape  from  the  poor,  enclosed,  drugged,  hopeless  world  in  which  his 
life  has  been  lived  -but  he  is  not  content  to  do  as  many  others  have,  to  gel 
his  skiils,  "go  make  his  pile"  and  separate  himself.  Another  student  wlio 
intends  to  be  a  lawyer,  commented,  "I  don't  look  for  big  hopes  anymore 
but  I've  learned  I  can  do  something  and  I'm  not  going  to  stop  fighting," 

These  are  the  people  who  can  provide  community  mental  health  serv- 
ices, legal  aid  and  instruction  from  within  their  communities.  These  are 
the  new  people  who  are  needed  in  the  mental  health  professions.  The 
attitude  of  these  students  offers  some  measure  of  the  urgent  need  for 
change.  One  girl  in  the  program,  who  had  been  quite  upset,  was  asked 
whether  she  would  like  to  see  a  campus  psychiatrist. 

"For  what?  I  have  had  two  of  those,  a  psychiatrist  and  a  social 
worker.  One  told  me  I  had  problems  with  authority  when  I  was  late 
for  an  appointment,  so  I  came  early  the  next  time  and  was  told  I  was 
too  anxious.  When  I  got  the  social  worker,  I  tried  to  be  exactly  on 
time  and  you  know  what  she  told  me?  I  was  compulsive.  How  do 
you  win  with  that  kind  of  closed  system?  The  only  thing  I  could  do 
is  drop  dead.  They  had  no  awareness  that  I  had  no  car,  had  to  beg  a 
ride  most  of  the  time,  and  had  no  money  all  of  the  time." 

Another  student  told  a  conference  of  mental  health  professions  ami 
social  workers,  "You  mean  well,  but  you  really  can't  help  with  the  kind  of 
problems  we  have." 

126 


Colleges  and  piofessional  schools  could  altei  this  impasse  by  training 
low-income  people  and  by  developing  techniques  for  working  with  the 
pool,  instead  of  sending  emissaries  by  day  to  help  the  pooi  adjust  to  being 
poor.  By  drawing  professionals  from  within  poor  communities,  social 
work  and  the  mental  health  professions  might  promote  community  activ- 
ity, patticipation  in  city  planning,  in  education,  and  in  the  establishment 
of  agencies  for  health  caie  and  housing.  Such  participation  would  enhance 
a  constitutive  process  of  change  that  is  a  matter  of  ethics  as  much  as 
money. 

It  has  been  estimated  that  some  200,000  youngsters  who  are  in  the  top 
of  the  U.S.  population  (judged  by  ability)  will  never  acquire  the  means  to 
go  to  college.  These  are  the  potential  leaders  who  could  rage  the  real  war 
against  poverty.  Operation  Hope  began  with  the  idea  that  the  selection 
and  training  of  young  leaders  from  the  ghetto  would  offer  a  model  for 
i  emitting  and  educating  a  new  kind  of  personnel  in  social  work  profes- 
sions. These  would  be  people  who  could  inject  the  energy  of  personal 
involvement  and  leality  understanding  into  helping  professions  such  as 
social  work. 

In  its  short  existence,  Operation  Hope  has  already  shown  that  there  are 
potential  leaders  within  the  ghetto,  whose  ideals  and  intellect  could  make 
them  the  agents  of  peaceful  social  change.  The  education  of  such  people  is 
difficult  because  it  must  compensate  for  so  much:  the  students'  lack  of 
family,  confidence,  gamesmanship,  academic  skills,  medical,  psycho- 
logical, and  legal  aid  -  in  addition  to  money.  No  program  to  aid  and 
educate  potential  young  talents  from  the  ghetto  can  realistically  meet 
these  needs,  nor  leave  them  to  the  universities  and  colleges.  The  ghetto 
environment  so  rapidly  destroys  trust  and  normal  human  emotions  that 
the  Operation  Hope  staff  has  begun  to  see  that  its  program  should  begin 
with  high  school  students.  No  person  can  cope  with  life  and  death  crises  in 
his  family  day  after  day  and  yet  devote  himself  to  learning,  yet  this  is 
what  is  expected  of  the  ghetto  student.  As  early  as  possible,  students 
should  be  relieved  of  some  of  the  reality  conflicts.  In  addition,  paucity  of 
funds  leaves  huge  gaps  in  their  education;  unlike  many  of  their  peers  they 
never  had  the  opportunity  to  relate  to  man's  universal  problems  through 
visual  art,  drama,  or  music.  Most  of  these  students  have  seen  little  of  the 
world  outside  their  neighborhoods.  Yet,  as  periodic  analyses  show,  they 
are  aware  of  the  culture  outside,  and  as  their  education  has  made  them 
more  perceptive,  self-confident,  and  articulate,  they  are  creating  art  of 
their  own. 

This  program  while  only  a  miniscule  experiment  in  the  overall  context 
of  America  today,  has  shown  that  a  remarkable  transformation  can  be 
made.  It  has  helped  to  liberate  a  new  kind  of  student.  Because  they  have 
experienced  more  of  life  than  most  men  of  50  when  they  arrive  in  col- 
lege, these  "youths"  are  not  passive  intellectuals.  They  approach  the  arts 
and  social  sciences  with  a  personal  sense  of  involvement  and  judgment. 
They  do  not  merely  accept;  they  question.  If  they  are  old  in  outlook, 
their  minds  are  capacious,  Their  brains  are  young.  They  have  vitality, 
compassion,  motivation.  The  result  is  a  kind  of  genius,  a  combination  of 

127 


wisdom  and  brightness,  a  depth  that  is  not  often  found.  Trivin  will  no( 
distract  them,  nor  will  minor  hardships  present  obstacles.  They  have  a 
highly  developed  sense  of  justice,  and  an  eagerness  for  history,  and  indeed, 
at  23  and  24,  they  resemble  the  idealistic  young  men  of  the  American 
Revolution  who,  at  the  same  age,  were  writing  the  Declaration  of  Inde- 
pendence and  attempting  to  fashion  a  constitution.  The  qualities  that  liavc 
been  elicited  in  these  men  and  women  include  a  kind  of  idealistic  willing- 
ness to  gamble  on  the  future.  Many  young  people  have  given  up,  and  fee! 
that  the  establishment  will  not  permit  changes  for  the  benefit  of  the 
left-out  Americans.  The  participants  of  Operation  Hope  have  no  ceitaiuly 
that  change  is  possible,  but  they  have  committed  themselves  to  tiy,  With 
their  nascent  social  genius  they  may  indeed  tiansform  the  helping  profes- 
sions from  ineffectual  stopgap  procedures,  to  a  realistic  confrontation  of 
the  social  problems  behind  individual  ills.  They  may  infuse  new  creativity 
and  strength  into  the  mental  health  piofessions  by  enteiing  as  a  new 
kind  of  professional  -  one  who  is  fearless  rather  than  cowed,  analytical 
rather  than  sentimental,  one  who  is  undeludecl  about  the  detailed  nature 
of  the  ghetto  and  incapable  of  forgetting  its  manner  of  shaping  people. 
Unlike  many  social  workers  in  today's  piofessional  schools,  these  people 
will  be  able  to  function  in  the  poorest  levels  of  society  as  well  as  among 
middle-class  professionals.  They  will  be  able  to  offer  the  kind  of  innova- 
tion that  stems  from  understanding  where  one  started  and  what  one's  ends 
are.  If  their  affluent  classmates  are  currently  unsure  of  where  they  aio 
going,  these  students  are  seizing  their  intellectual  honing  as  a  liberation, 
for  this  is  the  only  way  out  of  the  real  hopelessness  in  whicJi  they  have 
lived.  The  template  offered  by  Operation  Hope  is  there  to  be  extended 
and  copied  throughout  the  countiy, 

Training  Grant.  MH  11513 

Dates  of  Interviews.  September  and  October  1970 


128 


You  are  the  bows  from  which  your  children  as 
living  arrows  are  sent  forth. 


-Kahlil  Gibran 


Investigators: 

Jean  W.  MacFarlane,  Ph.D. 

John  A.  Clausen,  Ph.D. 

Institute  of  Human  Development 

University  of  California 

Berkeley,  California 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Introduction  and  Summary 

The  usual  way  of  studying  how  the  circumstances  of  childhood  affect 
the  characteristics  of  adulthood  is  to  start  with  the  grown  person  and  try 
to  work  back.  So-called  longitudinal  studies,  though,  begin  with  the  child. 
Three  of  these,  directed  by  the  Institute  of  Human  Development  of  the 
University  of  California,  Berkeley,  and  supported  recently  with  NIMH 
help,  are  now  approximately  40  years  old  and  probably  offer  the  richest 
collection  of  data  ever  assembled  on  human  beings  over  a  long  period. 

The  projects  have  attempted  to  answer  such  questions  as: 

•  Do  personality  and  intelligence  change  during  the  years  or  remain 
constant?  To  what  extent  are  they  related  to  a  person's  very  early  experi- 
ences? 

•  How  is  the  mental  health  of  an  adult  related  to  his  life  at  home  and 
to  other  influences  during  childhood  and  adolescence? 

•  "What  factors  contribute  to  an  adult's  attitudes,  achievement,  psycho- 
logical health? 

In  the  beginning  each  study  had  its  own  set  of  objectives: 
The  Guidance  Study  was  primarily  interested  in  personality  develop- 
ment. It  began  studying  its  subjects  as  infants.  There  were  252  of  them  - 
every  third  child  bom  in  Berkeley  over  an  18-month  period  beginning 
January  1928.  The  children  were  weighed,  measured,  tested,  interviewed, 
and  observed  at  various  times  through  their  eighteenth  year.  Special  atten- 
tion was  given  to  their  life  at  home  during  the  preschool  years.  Informa- 
tion about  them  was  obtained  also  from  their  parents,  brothers  and  sisters, 
teachers,  and  classmates,  At  30,  when  they  were  rearing  children  of  their 
own,  167  of  them  were  studied  again.  (The  project  got  its  name  from  one 
of  the  original  objectives:  to  learn  whether  or  not  psychological  guidance 
offered  to  parents  would  lead  to  better  mental  health  for  their  children  as 

131 


adults.)  The  project's  director  until  recently  was  Jean  Walker  MacFailuiw, 
Ph.D.,  who  is  now  Professor  of  Psychology  and  Research  Psychologist, 
emeritus,  at  the  Institute  of  Human  Development  and  is  still  working  on 
the  study.  The  present  director  is  Marjorie  P.  Honzik,  Ph.  D,,  Research 
Psychologist  and  Lecturer. in  Psychology. 

The  Berkeley  Growth  Study,  which  also  began  m  1 928,  has  been  pai  lie- 
ularly  interested  in  physical  and  mental  growth.  Its  original  sarnpla  com- 
prised 61  healthy  hospital-bom  babies,  who  were  studied  while  they  were 
still  in  the  hospital  and  then  every  month  until  they  were  15  months  old. 
After  that,  they  were  studied  every  3  months  until  they  were  3  years  old, 
then  eveiy  6  months  until  they  were  18.  They  were  examined  and  inter- 
viewed again  when  they  were  21,  26,  and  36  years  old.  The  sample  now 
numbers  54.  The  project's  dhector  is  Nancy  Bayley,  Ph.D.,  research 
psychologist. 

The  Oakland  Growth  Study  has  been  concerned  with  the  effect  of 
adolesence-the  physical  and  psychological  changes  occurring  then,  ;ui;l 
the  accompanying  attitudes  and  behavior-upon  later  life.  The  study  be- 
gan in  1 93 1  with  the  fifth  grade  pupils  of  five  Oakland,  California,  schools 
who  would  be  entering  the  same  junior  high  school.  There  were  200  of 
these.  They  were  studied  intensively-through  measurements,  tests,  obser- 
vation, self-reports,  ratings  by  classmates  and  teachers,  and  other  means- 
through  the  six  years  of  junior  and  senior  high  school.  At  graduation,  165 
were  still  in  the  group.  Follow-up  studies  made  15,  20,  and  26  years ta 
have  reached  as  many  as  123.  The  study  began  under  the  direction  of 
Harold  E.  Jones,  Ph.D.,  and  Herbert  R.  Stolz,  M.D.  Its  director  since  1%& 
has  been  John  A.  Clausen,  Ph.D.,  professor  of  sociology  and  lescarcli 
sociologist. 

Almost  all  of  the  subjects  were  white.  In  the  Guidance  Study,  though,  3 
percent  were  Negro,  a  proportion  representative  of  the  community's 
Negro  population  in  1928.  Though  all  socioeconomic  levels  were  repre- 
sented, the  families  of  the  subjects  were  predominantly  middle  class. 

In  some  respects-in  a  ntliro  pome  tries,  intelligence  tests,  certain  person- 
ality measures-the  three  studies  overlapped,  making  it  possible  for  one  set 
of  findings  to  be  compared  to  another.  In  some  of  the  analyses  now  being 
made,  data  from  more  than  one  study  are  used.  AH  the  subjects,  it  is 
planned,  will  be  followed  through  life,  with  the  principal  research  interest 
fmm  nmw  nn  i^g  factors  connected  with  the  aging  process. 


particular,  much  of  that  obtained  during  the  most  recent  follow-ups— is 
still  being  analyzed. 

This  report  is  concerned  with  the  most  recent  findings  (roughly,  those 
of  the  past  five  years),  which  have  a  number  of  implications  for  mental 
health.  For  example,  findings  of  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study  indicate  that 
the  level  of  intelligence  continues  to  rise  until  at  least  the  age  of  36.  This 
was  true  for  persons  with  relatively  low  IQ's  as  well  as  for  those  with 
relatively  high  IQ's,  and  for  persons  from  all  socioeconomic  levels.  The  re- 
sults suggest  that,  on  the  average,  adults  reach  their  peak  of  mental  ability 
at  a  later  age  than  has  often  been  supposed.  This  age  for  the  group  under 
study  will  be  determined  in  future  follow-ups.  But  the  work  already  bears 
out  what  individuals  often  discover  for  themselves:  people  from  whatever 
level  retain  for  many  years  the  basic  brainpower  to  learn  new  ways,  new 
skills.  The  study  also  points  to  a  number  of  environmental  factors  in  very 
early  life-parental  attitudes  among  them—that  seem  to  influence  a  per- 
son's IQ  for  years  afterward  and  perhaps  permanently.  When  knowledge 
of  these  factors  is  widely  held  and  applied,  the  mental  competency  of  our 
population  can  be  expected  to  rise. 

By  studying  the  same  people  over  such  long  periods,  the  investigators 
have  also  been  able  to  demonstrate  that  the  mental  well-being  of  adults- 
those  in  their  thirties,  at  any  rate—is  related  to  certain  childhood  charac- 
teristics and  even  to  events  during  infancy.  Those  people  who  came  down 
with  psychosomatic  ailments  in  adulthood,  or  became  mentally  ill,  or 
showed  other  evidence  of  psychological  trouble  had  been  reporting  worse 
than  average  adjustments  to  life  even  as  1 1 -year-olds.  And  when  these 
individuals  were  less  than  2  years  old,  their  families  had  shown  more  than 
average  amount  of  disturbance.  One  of  the  investigators  reports  that  a 
major  factor  in  the  onset  of  psychosomatic  illness,  (which  may  include  to 
some  extent  almost  all  illness)  seems  to  be  a  tendency  towaid  depression 
reaching  far  back  into  childhood. 

Looking  into  the  records  of  their  subjects  as  children,  the  investigators 
have  also  identified  certain  characteristics  held  in  common  by  those  who 
later  took  up  smoking  and  continued  to  smoke.  And  the  person  most 
likely  to  become  a  problem  drinker,  preliminary  evidence  suggests,  could 
have  been  spotted  while  he  was  still  in  high  school. 

Most  of  the  recent  findings  should  be  useful  in  detecting  children  who 
need  special  help  if  they  are  able  to  realize  their  intellectual  potential  and 
be  able  to  lead  a  satisfying  life.  But  the  Guidance  Study  staff  warns  that  in 
predicting  at  18  a  person's  psychological  health  at  30  it  is  easy  to  over- 
emphasize the  effect  of  traumatic  experiences  and  of  the  youngster's 
response  to  them.  Many  of  the  most  stable  young  adults  were  those  for 
whom  the  staff  had  forecast  a  poor  outcome.  Disturbing  experiences  and 
behavior  during  adolescence  had  apparently  been,  in  these  cases,  maturing. 
Dr.  MacFarlane  concludes  that  pediatricians  and  other  doctors  should  look 
for  strengths  in  children  and  not  just  pathology. 

On  the  other  hand,  many  of  the  Guidance  Study  youngsters  who  had 
been  highly  popular  in  high  school  failed  to  live  up  to  their  potential-by 
the  age  of  30,  anyway-and  were  puzzled  and  dissatisfied,  perhaps  because 

133 


life  had  been  too  easy  for  them  or  because  they  had  poured  their  adoles- 
cent eneigies  into  maintaining  an  image  of  success. 

Some  of  the  other  findings   covered   in  the   following  sections  are 
summed  up  here: 

•  Certain  personality  traits  become  established  early  and  persist,  The 
child  who  at  five  was  either  reserved  and  shy  or  expressive  and  gay  tended 
to  show  the  same  characteiistics  at  16.  The  child  who  was  either  reaclne 
and  explosive  or  calm  and  phlegmatic  at  five  was  likely  to  be  the  same  at 
16. 

•  Personality  is  correlated  with  intelligence,  at  least  in  the  case  of  boys. 
Adolescent  boys  with  high  IQ's  were  generally  described  as  friendly , 
social,  and  independent-as  they  had  been  since  the  age  of  four.  There  was 
a  positive  relationship  between  these  characteristics  and  the  IQ  in  thease 
of  girls,  too,  but  it  was  not  statistically  significant.  Men  of  high  inlclli- 
gence  were  less  likely  than  men  of  low  intelligence  to  be  hostile,  self- 
pitying,  or  impatient. 

•  Frequently  the  effects  of  a  given  environmental  circumstance  art 
quite  different  for  one  sex  than  for  the  other  (as  they  are  for  subjects  witJi 
different  tempeiaments).  The  differences  are  found  in  the  devolopmeii! 
of  both  intelligence  and  personality. 

•  The  speed  of  development  during  childhood,  which  presumably  is 
determined  by  both  genetic  and  environmental  factors,  seems  to  influence 
peisonality  characteristics  into  adulthood.  The  early  talkers  (generally 
those  who  had  received  more  than  the  usual  amount  of  parental  attention 
during  infancy)  were  more  introspective   as  adults,  perhaps  because 
language  rather  than  action  had  always  been  for  them  the  favored  re- 
sponse pattern.  Boys  who  matured  early  had  an  easier  time  during  adoles- 
cence, a  highei  income  at  30,  and  a  strong  tendency  to  conform. 


How  the  Early  Environment  Affects  Intellectual  Development 

A  child's  experiences  during  his  very  early  years  seem  to  affect  tlie  lowl 
of  his  intelligence  for  at  least  many  years  afterward.  The  main  conclusions 
on  this  subject,  summarized  here,  stem  from  analyses  either  by  Uoiirik 
and  her  associates  in  the  Guidance  Study  or  by  Bayley  and  her  associates 
in  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study. 

Socioeconomic  conditions.  The  socioeconomic  level  of  the  parents  was 
unrelated  to  the  intelligence  of  their  children  during  the  first  IS  or  24 
months.  But  after  that,  and  particularly  after  the  age  of  five  years,  the 
relationship  became  fairly  strong:  in  general,  the  lower  the  parents' status 
when  the  child  was  young,  the  lower  the  children's  intelligence  as  mew- 
ured  by  mental  tests.  This  was  true  through  at  least  the  age  of  18,  which  is 
as  far  as  this  part  of  the  analysis  has  gone. 

Family  income  during  the  early  preschool  years  was  significantly  re- 
lated to  the  children's  scores  on  mental  tests  at  most  points  up  through 
the  age  of  15.  At  the  age  of  30,  it  was  significantly  related  to  the  sons' 
scores  but  not  the  daughters'.  Superior  play  facilities  during  early  cfirW- 
hood  were  also  related  to  the  IQ  scores  later  on. 


Parental  attitudes  toward  education.  Both  boys  and  girls  whose  parents 
had  expressed  concern  when  the  children  weie  babies  that  they  get  a  good 
education  were  likely  to  make  higher  IQ  scores  at  all  ages  through  30  than 
the  children  of  parents  who  had  shown  little  or  no  concern.  Boys  were 
motivated  by  their  mothers'  concern;  girls,  by  their  fathers'. 

Parents'  marital  adjustment.  When  the  home  atmosphere  very  early  in 
life  had  been  one  of  parental  harmony,  or  at  least  of  lack  of  conflict,  a 
girl's  mental  test  performance— but  not  a  boy's—was  likely  to  be  signifi- 
cantly bettei  right  on  up  into  adulthood. 

Maternal  characteristics.  Mothers  who  appeared  worrisome,  tense, 
highly  active,  and  energetic  had  childien  who  were  more  likely  than  other 
children  to  score  high  on  mental  tests  through  the  age  of  30.  The  child's 
need  for  tactual,  auditory,  and  visual  stimulation,  Honzik  notes,  aie  best 
met  in  the  family  where  the  mother  is  icsponsivc  and  actively  concerned 
with  the  infant's  welfare,  even  to  the  point  of  being  worrisome  about  him. 
A  genetic  factor  may  be  at  work,  too,  for  the  worrisome  mother  tended 
also  to  be  the  better  educated  mother,  and  educational  level  in  this 
sample,  the  investigators  think,  was  evidence  of  native  ability  as  well  as  of 
stimulation  during  childhood. 

For  the  boys,  the  one  best  predictor  of  test  peiformance  during  the 
period  between  8  and  1 8  was  the  closeness  of  the  mother-son  relationship 
as  rated  at  21  months.  Apparently,  it  is  verbal  competence  that  is  fostered 
by  a  close  lelationship.  For  at  1 8,  a  boy's  verbal  IQ  was  much  more  likely 
to  be  high  if  the  early  relationship  had  been  close;  his  performance  IQ  was 
unaffected.  At  30,  when  the  test  was  for  performance,  not  verbal  ability, 
there  was  no  correlation. 

Boys  whose  mothers  were  anxious,  irritable,  strict,  and  punitive  toward 
them  during  their  first  few  years  tended  to  have  IQ's  below  average  during 
the  school  years  and  even  as  adults.  Maternal  love  or  lack  of  it  seemed  to 
have  no  effect  on  the  mental  test  scores  of  girls.  But  mothers  judged  to  be 
instrusive -forever  meddling  in  the  child's  activities-had  daughters  whose 
IQ  ratings  through  adolescence  tended  to  be  low. 

Paternal  characteristics.  A  girl's  intellectual  development  was  likely  to 
be  increased  when,  during  babyhood,  her  father  had  a  close,  warm  rela- 
tionship with  both  his  wife  and  with  her.  It  was  the  father's  interest  in  his 
daughter,  rather  than  his  expression  of  affection  for  her,  that  seemed  to 
count.  The  father-daughter  relationship  apparently  influenced  IQ  scores 
from  the  age  of  seven  through  adolescence.  During  the  preschool  years, 
the  relationship  between  mother  and  daughter  was  the  more  influential. 

Childhood  IQ  and  adult  success.  In  the  case  of  men  the  IQ  through 
childhood  and  adolescence  is  roughly  a  good  indication  of  their  success- 
conventionally  judged-as  an  adult.  On  the  basis  of  the  number  of  years 
they  had  gone  to  school  and  on  the  kind  of  jobs  they  held  at  the  age  of 
30,  the  men  seemed  to  be  achieving  "pretty  much  in  accord  with  their 
mental  abilities."  This  was  not  true  of  the  women.  But  then,  Bayley 
points  out,  women  in  general  have  different  educational  and  occupational 

135 


goals  than  men.  As  a  group  the  women  who  classified  themselves  as  house- 
wives had  had,  all  their  lives,  higher  IQ's  than  the  women  who  were 
working  for  pay. 

The  influence  of  sex.  As  noted  in  the  findings  above,  certain  factor* 
that  influenced  the  IQ's  of  boys  had  little  or  no  influence  on  the  IQ's  of 
girls,  and  vice  versa.  Bayley  suggests  there  is  a  genetic  difference  between 
the  sexes  in  the  ability  to  resist  certain  environmental  influences  or  to 
recover  from  them.  "Boys,"  she  adds,  "appear  to  be  less  able  than  girls  to 
recover  from  hostile,  rejecting  treatment;  but  they  may  also  profit  mow, 
in  the  long  run,  from  understanding  loving  acceptance."  There  also  scenii 
to  be,  Honzik  reports,  a  sex  difference  in  the  rate  of  mental  growth,  Tfic 
parents'  education  showed  an  increasing  correlation  with  the  children's  IQ's 
between  21  months  and  1 5  years,  but  it  became  significant  for  girls  by  tit 
age  of  three;  for  boys,  not  until  five.  When  there  were  boys  and  girls  in  Hie 
same  family,  this  difference  still  held.  The  lelationship  between  socio- 
economic  status  and  IQ  also  became  significant  for  girls  earlier  than  For 
boys.  Girls'  abilities,  Honzik  suggests,  develop  earlier. 

The  home  environment  in  later  years.  The  family  situation  was  assessed 
each  year  until  the  children  were  1 6,  In  general,  the  relationships  between 
a  given  factor,  such  as  parental  warmth,  on  the  one  hand,  and  menial 
development  on  the  other  were  less-if  they  were  present  at  all-lliai 
they  had  been  during  early  childhood.  In  other  words,  the  family  situation 
when  the  child  is  very  young  influences  mental  development  mow 
strongly  than  the  situation  later  on,  when  out-of-family  influences  in- 
crease. 


The  Course  of  the  IQ  as  People  Grow  Older 

After  the  1964  follow-up  of  members  of  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study, 
Bayley  began  analyzing  the  IQ's  of  the  subjects  at  that  time,  when  they 
were  36  years  old,  in  relation  to  some  of  the  earlier  IQ's-those  shown  at 
the  ages  of  16,  18,  21,  and  26  years.  She  finds  that  until  the  ageof36,al 
least,  the  intelligence  level  tends  to  rise. 

This  is  particularly  true  with  the  verbal  scale  of  the  test,  which  meas- 
ures the  extent  of  a  person's  vocabulary  and  information,  his  ability  to 
comprehend,  and  other  factors  heavily  dependent  on  his  capacity  to 
understand  and  use  words.  On  this  scale,  all  the  subjects,  show  an  increase 
in  score  with  age,  between  16  and  36,  though  the  rate  of  increase  ii 
slowing  down. 

On  the  performance  scale  of  the  intelligence  test,  there  is  a  slight 
decline  after  26.  The  men  score  the  same  at  36  as  at  26,  but  the  women 
score  lower.  This  scale  deals  with  the  ability  to  perceive  patterns,  to 
visualize  the  whole  from  some  of  its  parts,  and  to  manipulate  objects  in  a 
logical  way. 

As  a  group,  Bayley  points  out,  the  subjects  in  the  study  have  had  a 
better  socioeconomic  background  than  average  and  have  scored  a  bow 

136 


average  in  the  mental  tests.  However,  the  group  does  include  several  per- 
sons whose  scores  during  childhood  were  quite  low,  and  these  persons, 
too,  have  continued  to  grow  in  intelligence. 

The  person  with  the  lowest  IQ  was  a  man  whose  ratings  were  in  the  low 
60's  from  the  time  he  was  5  until  16.  After  that  they  went  up,  and  at  36 
reached  80.  His  performance  IQ  then  was  92;  his  verbal  IQ,  72.  He  had 
not  learned  to  read  until  after  he  was  21 . 

Bayley  concludes  that  the  intellectual  potential  of  the  people  in  her 
study  remain  unimpaired  through  36  years  and  that  "in  the  attainment  of 
information  and  word  knowledge  their  intelligence  is  continuing  to  in- 
crease." She  finds  some  evidence— the  lowered  speed  with  which  the 
women  in  general  completed  some  of  the  tests  on  the  performance  scale- 
consistent  with  the  findings  of  other  investigators  that  advancing  age  is 
accompanied  by  loss  of  speed  in  learning.  However,  she  points  out  that 
other  investigatois  have  found  evidence  also  that  loss  of  speed  is  often 
compensated  for  by  an  increase  in  knowledge  and  skill. 

As  people  grow  older,  this  investigator  believes,  they  probably  show 
increasing  resistance  to  learning  new  techniques  and  new  ways  of  organ- 
izing knowledge,  but  the  extent  to  which  these  resistances  are  overcome 
may  be  matters  of  motivation  and  opportunity,  rather  than  of  intelli- 
gence. 

She  points  to  two  men  in  the  study  who  went  back  to  school  in  their 
thirties,  after  a  decade  in  other  employment,  one  to  become  a  lawyer  and 
the  other  a  doctor.  Both  have  started  their  new  careers  "with  bright 
prospects  of  success."  She  concludes  that  motivation  and  drive  and  ample 
time,  rather  than  a  small  variation  in  intelligence,  seem  to  be  "the  im- 
portant determiners  for  much  of  learning  in  adults." 

Do  smart  babies  become  smart  children  and  smart  adults? 

Not  necessarily, 

Bayiey  and  some  of  her  associates  have  been  analyzing  the  correlations 
between  mental  test  scores  in  infancy  (the  average  of  several  scores  at  10 
to  12  months)  with  test  scores  at  24  ages,  running  from  1  month  to  36 
years.  For  the  first  2  or  3  years  the  relationships  are  close;  then  they  fall 
off  rapidly.  From  4  years  on,  there  is  very  little  relationship,  particularly 
in  the  case  of  the  boys. 

The  investigators  have  also  developed  "precocity  scores"  in  order  to 
observe  how  the  precocious  infants  fared  later  on.  These  scores  are  based 
on  the  ages  at  which  a  child  first  put  a  block  together,  first  noticed  a 
pellet  on  the  table,  first  said  a  syllable  that  had  meaning  for  adults,  and  so 
on.  There  are  1 1 5  items  in  all. 

So  far  the  analysis  has  been  completed  only  for  the  vocabulary  or 
vocalization  factor,  comprising  seven  items  or  steps  that  the  normal  child 
generally  completes  between  the  ages  of  8  and  15  months.  These  include 
expressing  emotions  with  distinctive  sounds  and,  later  on,  saying  words. 
During  the  first  3  years  of  life,  the  higher  the  score  on  this  factor,  the 
higher  the  IQ,  for  both  sexes.  Then  the  boys'  correlations  drop  sharply 
and  in  most  cases  become  negative,  meaning  that  the  higher  the  score  on 
the  vocalization  factor,  the  lower  the  IQ.  For  the  girls,  however,  the 

137 


correlations  remain  liigh:  in  their  case  vocal  precocity  as  a  baby  is  a 
prediction  of  high  IQ  as  children  and  as  adults. 


Intelligence  as  Related  to  Behavior  and  Personality 

The  follow-up  of  the  people  in  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study  when  they 
were  36  included  a  detailed  personality  assessment  made  on  the  basis  of 
the  100-item  Block  Q-sort.  (In  a  Q-sort,  characteristics  noted  during  an 
interview  are  given  numerical  weight  by  scoring  each  item  on  a  scale.  The 
interviewer  thus  can  say  that  the  given  quality  was  absent,  present  loa 
veiy  high  degree,  or  present  to  one  of  the  several  degrees  in  between,  In 
this  study  the  transcribed  interviews  were  Q-sor£ed  by  the  interviewer  and 
by  two  clinical  psychologists.)  Bayley  has  now  analyzed  the  findings  and 
compared  them  with  the  scores  made  on  IQ  tests  during  the  same  follow 
up. 

The  men  who  had  been  scored  high  on  such  items  as  impatient,  negatfr 
istic,  self-pitying,  and  hostile  were  found  to  be  those  who  in  general  tori 
the  lowest  IQY  The  men  described  as  critical,  introspective,  socially  per- 
ceptive, and  having  wide  interests  were  those  in  general  with  the  highest 
IQ's.  Little  or  no  relationship  was  found  between  IQ  scores  on  the  one 
hand,  and  on  the  other  hand,  characteristics  described  as  either  distant 
and  avoiding  or  warm,  calm,  and  gregarious. 

"The  men  m  this  sample  with  high  intelligence,"  Bayley  reports,  "arc 
best  characterized  as  intiospective,  thoughtful,  and  concerned  with  prob- 
lems, meanings  and  values;  they  are  men  who  are  perceptive  and  have  a 
wide  lange  of  interests.  The  least  intelligent  are  most  often  found  to  be 
impatient,  prone  to  vent  their  hostilities  and  to  project  them  onto 
others." 

The  correlations  between  the  women's  [Q's  and  various  personality 
attributes  are  much  weaker  but  similar  in  pattern.  Again  it  is  the  thought- 
ful, insightful  person  with  wide  interests  who  is  more  likely  to  score  high 
on  the  IQ  tests.  Women  described  as  bland,  conventional,  or  anxious  arc 
much  less  likely  to  rate  high  on  the  tests;  so  are  women  described  :is 
cheerful,  poised,  and  gregarious. 

Another  measuie  of  personality  styles  and  psychological  attitudes 
administered  during  this  follow-up  was  the  California  Psychological  Inven- 
tory, a  questionnaire  designed  to  measure  such  characteristics  as  sociabil- 
ity, self-acceptance,  sense  of  well-being,  tolerance,  and  responsibility.  II 
has  17  scales.  Bayley  has  compared  the  scores  made  on  each  of  these  at 
the  age  of  36  with  the  IQ  scores  at  that  age  and  also  at  1 6,  1 8,  21 ,  and  26 
years. 

Certain  of  these  characteristics  appear  to  be  significantly  associated 
with  a  high  order  of  intelligence  at  all  the  ages  studied.  For  men,  the 
clearest  and  most  consistent  associations  with  IQ's  are  socialization  (refer- 
ring to  social  maturity,  integrity,  rectitude);  the  ability  to  make  a  good 
impression;  potential  for  achievement,  whether  by  conforming  with  the 
group  or  acting  independently;  and  intellectual  efficiency.  For  women,  the 

138 


cleaiest  and  most  consistent  associations  aie  with  tolerance,  potential  for 
achievement  by  acting  independently,  and  flexibility. 

Ratings  on  self-acceptance  and  self-control,  qualities  usually  associated 
with  mental  health,  were  not  significantly  related  to  intelligence  in  cither 
men  or  women. 

When  Bayley  used  the  scores  on  the  subscales  of  the  intelligence  test, 
she  found  some  other  provocative  patterns.  Little  or  no  sex  difference 
appeared  in  the  correlations  between  scores  on  the  verbal-academic  scales 
of  the  IQ  tests  and  the  ratings  for  achievement  potential,  intellectual 
efficiency,  and  inteiest  in  intellectual  pursuits.  But  the  other  scales 
pointed  to  marked  differences.  With  men  but  not  with  women,  for  ex- 
ample, the  score  on  the  picture-completion  test  correlated  strongly  with 
the  score  on  socialization;  with  women  but  not  with  men  it  correlated 
with  flexibility.  Scores  on  the  object-assembly  test  correlated  with  flexi- 
bility, achievement  potential,  and  intellectual  efficiency  with  the  women 
but  not  with  the  men.  The  highest  scores  in  arithmetic  were  made  by  the 
women  rating  highest  in  feminine  qualities  and  by  the  men  rating  lowest 
in  them.  The  highest  scores  in  the  digit-span  test  were  made  by  the  men 
ranking  high  in  sociability,  well-being,  and  interest  in  making  a  good  im- 
pression and  by  the  women  ranking  low  in  these  characteristics. 

In  short,  for  this  small  sample  at  least,  there  seems  to  be  a  relationship 
between  intellectual  processes  and  personality  as  manifested  in  various 
social  attitudes,  interests,  and  motivations.  The  lelationship  remains  fairly 
stable  over  the  years  between  16  and  36,  but  differs  both  with  the  nature 
of  the  intellectual  piocess  and  with  the  personality  characteristic  being 
considered.  It  often  differs  widely  between  the  sexes  as  well. 

In  the  case  of  males,  the  investigators  have  also  found  some  relations 
between  mental  test  scores  throughout  the  36-year  period  and  the  be- 
havior and  personality  characteristics  of  the  subjects  dining  their  first 
three  years.  Boys  who  were  calm,  responding,  and  happy,  and  who  were 
active  after  15  months  rather  than  before,  weie  more  likely  than  the 
others  to  have  high  IQ's  (determined  in  this  case  only  from  the  verbal 
scale).  Girls  showed  no  clear  pattern. 

With  females  through  the  years,  considerably  fewer  significant  correla- 
tions between  IQ  scores  and  personality  latings  were  noted  than  foi  males. 
Bayley  suggests  that  a  girl's  intellectual  potential  is  less  affected  than  a 
man's  by  social  and  emotional  factors.  A  girl  comes  into  life  physio- 
logically tougher,  it  has  been  shown;  perhaps  she  is  by  nature  psycho- 
logically tougher  as  well. 


Psychological  Mechanisms  and  the  IQ 

After  the  subjects  in  the  Oakland  Growth  Study  had  been  interviewed 
at  37,  Norma  Haan,  a  psychologist,  rated  them  for  the  presence  of  coping 
and  defense  mechanisms.  Among  the  coping  mechanisms  she  includes 
objectivity,  logical  analysis,  empathy,  sublimation,  and  tolerance  of 
ambiguity.  Among  the  defense  mechanisms  are  repression,  doubt  and 

139 


indecision,  and  denial  of  facts  and  feelings  that  would  be  unpleasant 
self-threatening  to  acknowledge.  The  coping  and  defense  mechanisms 
counterpaits.  For  example,  the  coping  partner  of  denial,  says  Haan. 
concentration-the  ability  to  set  aside  disturbing  feelings  or  thoughts 
order  to  get  on  with  necessary  tasks  at  hand, 

In  general,  the  adults  who  tended  to  make  use  of  coping  rather  th 
defense  mechanisms  had  the  highest  iQ's.  Further,  they  were  the  pcrso 
whose  IQ's  between  adolescence  and  adulthood  were  most  likely  to  ha 
risen.  Coping,  the  investigator  suggests,  leads  to  the  development  of  om 
intelligence;  defensiveness  interfeies  with  one's  intelligence  as  well  as  one 
effectiveness. 


Persistence  of  Personality  Traits 

Dr.  Wanda  C.  Bronson,  a  psychologist,  has  begun  to  analyze  the  alt 
tudes  and  characteristics  of  subjects  in  the  Guidance  Study  to  learn  i 
these  become  set  very  early  or  change  with  the  years, 

Between  the  ages  of  5  and  1 6,  the  period  covered  by  the  analysis  so  fa; 
she  finds  two  persistent  "behavioral  dimensions."  One  is  behavior  ctac 
terized  at  one  end  of  the  dimension  as  reserved,  somber,  shy,  and  at  th 
other  end  as  expressive,  gay,  socially  easy.  The  second  dimension  is  a 
contrast  between  reactive,  explosive,  resistive  behavior  at  one  end-calm. 
phlegmatic,  compliant  behavior  at  the  other  end. 

If  a  child  was  either  reserved  (somber,  shy)  or  expressive  (gay,  social!) 
easy)  at  the  age  of  five,  he  was  likely  to  be  the  same  at  1 6.  If  he  was  cttht 
reactive  (explosive,  resistive)  or  calm  (phlegmatic,  compliant)  at  five,  IK 
was  likely  to  be  the  same  at  1 6. 

The  reserved  individual  tended  also  to  be  introspective  and,  to  a  lesser 
extent,  anxious  and  socially  withdrawn.  In  early  childhood,  he  was  likely 
to  be  inactive  and  a  poor  eater;  at  16,  uncertain  and  uncompetitive,  Tk 
expressive  boy  was  the  extrovert.  Girls  showed  only  one  marked  dif- 
ference from  the  boys  on  this  behavior  measure.  The  reserved  girl  leixled 
to  be  cautious  and  imadventurous  at  all  ages  whereas  the  reserved  boy  wis 
cautious  and  imadventurous  only  between  the  ages  of  8  and  1 0. 

In  the  case  of  the  other  behavior  grouping,  the  reactive  and  explosive 
boy  tended  also  to  be  emotionally  unstable,  quarrelsome,  and  complain- 
ing. In  adolescence  but  not  earlier  he  was  also  likely  to  be  rated  active  an! 
adventurous.  Girls  showed  some  differences.  The  correlation  between 
reactiveness  and  emotional  instability  was  not  significantly  strong  exwpl 
during  early  childhood;  the  correlations  between  reactiveness  and  the 
activity  level,  strong  between  the  ages  of  8  and  13.  The  reactive  giil> 
tended  to  be  finicky  about  their  food  at  all  ages  and  to  be  exhibitionist 
at  16. 

Both  of  these  attitude  patterns -reserve  v,  expressiveness  and  rettctivily 
v,  placidity^describe  characteristics  that  the  individual  brings  with  htoilo 
every  situation  and  that  affect  the  environment's  impact  upon  him.  An 
expressive  child,  for  example,  would  be  more  ready  to  initiate  or  be  dram 

140 


into  an  intensive  relation  with  his  mothei  than  a  withdrawn  child.  A  reac- 
tive child,  more  than  a  placid  one,  would  be  affected  by  an  anxious, 
intrusive  mother. 

To  what  extent  and  through  the  mediation  of  what  mechanisms  these 
petsistent  personality  traits  are  inherited,  affected  by  the  environment, 
and  developed  in  the  interaction  between  heredity  and  environment  is  not 
cleai.  Bronson  does  find  that  the  children  tend  to  take  after  the  parent  of 
the  same  sex  and  to  reject  or  be  unaffected  by  the  characteristics  of  the 
other  parent.  Expressive  boys,  for  example,  tended  to  have  fathers  of 
expressive  and  even  aggressive  temperament;  expressive  girls,  mothers  of 
the  same  type.  More  information  on  this  question  is  expected  to  come 
from  an  analysis  of  the  children  of  the  subjects  in  the  study. 

In  another  of  the  Berkeley  studies,  the  boys  with  the  loving  mothers— 
these  were  the  boys  most  likely  to  have  high  IQ's  later  on-tended  as 
babies  to  be  happy,  inactive,  and  slow,  Beginning  about  the  age  of  4, 
though,  these  boys  were  consistently  rated  as  independent,  social,  and 
friendly. 

Personality  as  Related  to  Speed  of  Development 

Some  of  the  personality  traits  noted  in  preceding  sections  seem  to  be 
related  to  the  late  at  which  the  people  developed— began  talking,  began 
walking,  reached  adolescence.  The  early  talkers,  Dr.  MacFarlane  reports, 
tended  to  be  the  late  walkers.  And  those  who  matured  late,  as  indicated 
by  the  age  they  reached  pubescence,  differed  considerably  in  some  re- 
spects during  adolescence  and  young  adulthood  from  those  who  matured 
early.  This  was  true  of  boys  in  particular. 

As  an  example  of  how  the  rate  of  physical  development  in  youth  can 
influence  a  person's  life  for  years  afterward,  this  investigator  tells  of  two 
boys  who  differed  mainly  in  speed  of  maturation.  The  early  maturer  (who 
reached  adult  sexual  status  before  he  was  13)  excelled  in  athletics  and 
enjoyed  the  accompanying  rewards.  He  showed  interest  in  girls  at  13  and 
they  in  him.  The  late  maturer  (who  reached  adult  sexual  status  after  he 
was  17)  avoided  girls,  and  they  him,  till  he  was  20.  The  first  boy  got  a 
summer  job  at  14;  the  second  went  to  Boy  Scout  camp.  After  college,  the 
early  maturer  joined  a  firm  in  another  city,  married,  and  by  30  had 
reached  a  responsible  position  that  takes  him  and  his  family  to  all  parts  of 
the  world.  The  late  maturer  married  a  girl  he  had  known  since  grade 
school,  got  a  promising  job  with  her  father's  help,  and  established  a  home 
in  the  neighborhood  where  they  had  been  born  and  raised.  At  30  he  had 
yet  to  reach  the  administrative  level  in  his  firm.  "I'm  too  young,  they  tell 
me,"  he  reported.  "I've  always  been  too  young." 

Though  the  details  differ  from  person  to  person,  the  early  maturing 
male  has  been  found  by  the  Guidance  Study  to  have  an  easier  time  during 
adolescence  and  to  show  more  confidence  both  then  and  later.  Tliis  was  to 
have  been  expected,  MacFarlane  thinks,  because  the  boy  who  matured 
early  was  also  likely  to  have  begun  early  getting  into  things  and  exploring 
the  environment.  His  interests  were  outward  because  there's  where  the 


141 


excitement  lay.  On  the  other  hand,  the  late  maturcr  wus 
early  talker,  an  intioveited  fellow  who  was  appaicntly  IIKHO 
fiom  the  start  by  thinking  pioccsses  than  by  action. 

At  30,  the  early  matuier  was  likely  to  have  advanced  I'mllkT  i> 
than  the  late  maturei,  more  likely  to  be  married,  and  likely  (o  I 
children. 

Findings  by  Dr.  Mary  Covei   Jones,  Piofessor  of  liduejiltoii, 
from  her  study  of  a  different  sample-early  and  late  muUiiinn  b( 
Oakland  Giowth  Study-confirm  those  results,  add  scum1  inlet1 
tails,  and  cany  the  comparison  a  little  farther  along.  A|  ,13.  llu" 
had  matured  early  rated  significantly  higher  in  both  souiihiliC 
sponsibility.  They  were  also  more  conventional   in   then   ,i[(j( 
thinking.  Five  years  later  the  diffeienccs  were  less  marked,  but 
who  had  matured  early  still  appeared  to  be  moic  assured  ami  s 
less  fearful,  and  also  less  insightful  and  independent.  Mine  ot 
maturing  men  have  attained  executive,  status-conferring',  vocii1ci» 

The  boy  who  most  rapidly  approaches  physical  mtmhoml,  J 
gests,  is  the  one  who  is  first  recognized  by  the  adult  community 
therefoie  is  most  likely  to  take  on-if  he  doesn't  have  I  hem  .tin 
peisonalily  traits  likely  to  be  most  valued  by  that  eomimirnly.  J)i 
Peskin,  a  psychologist,  thinks  the  difference  found  in  holli  sin 
well  have  a  deep  psychological  basis.  The  early  matuier,  IK-  MI^II^ 
prepared  for  the  changes  of  adolescence.  So  he  may  expi-iieiKv 
less  tolerable  and  theiefore  less  acceptable.  He  flees,  lheiotnK\  in 
hood  and  makes  an  early  and  rewarding  commitment  to  [\w  \.ili 
cultme.  So  he  is  "naturally"  more  sociable  and  conformhii'.  On  t 
hand,  the  late  maturer,  not  having  to  deal  with  the  hoiiiuiiul 
duves  till  later,  has  more  time  to  look  around,  expand  his  st, 
develop  a  vaiiety  of  psychological  mechanisms  for  ivfuiLiluip  i 
when  puberty  comes,  he  tolerates  it  bcttei  and  has  less  mv»l  ol 
supports  and  rewards.  This  would  explain  his  greater  msii'liUulm-*- 

Here  again  an  apparent  sex  difference  lias  been  I'oniul,  <i 
leached  maturity  early,  MacFailane  lepoits,  were  usually  hv»  onii 
adults.  In  school  they  seem  to  have  fell  out  of  thini's,  Tltvv,- 
described  by  Mary  Jones,  on  the  basis  of  obscivational  uiliu^.  .^  ' 
disadvantaged,"  However,  their  responses  on  the  Thematic  API<, 
Test  and  their  self-report  scores  indicate  adequate  scll'-coiuvpl  s 

Dr.  Louis  Stewart  adds  this  finding:  Among  males,  :it  Ir.^i,  l 
bom  tends  to  mature  earlier  than  an  only  child  or  a  hisllmm  Ju 
psychologist  thinks  the  earlier  maturation  is  somehow  assuci.ikd  i 
events  attendant  on  the  arrival  of  a  new  baby,  for  when  ,i  moi 
pregnant  with  her  second  child,  and  for  a  year  or  two  alU'r  iK  lv 
fhstboin  showed  an  unusual  spurt  in  growth.  Numerous 
animals  and  some  studies  of  people  show  that  stimulation  in 
painful  stimulation,  makes  for  growth,  and  separation  lioiti 
appears  to  be  an  important  form  of  such  stimulation.  The 
nancy  and  the  arrival  of  a  new  child,  the  investigator  suun 
painful,  development-spurring  stimulation  for  the  firsthnin. 

142 


Incidentally,  othei  work  on  buth  oicler  by  Dr  William  T.  Smelser,  also 
a  psychologist,  throws  new  light  on  the  recuncnt  finding  that  firstborn 
children  get  moic  education  than  those  born  last.  In  two-child  families 
where  both  children  aie  of  the  same  sex,  Sinelsci  finds,  there  is  no  signifi- 
cant diffeience  in  the  number  of  years  they  go  to  school.  But  where  one 
child  is  a  boy  and  the  other  is  a  gill,  the  fiistborn,  of  whichever  sex,  goes 
to  school  significantly  longer  And  theic  is  a  greater  pioportion  of  these 
cross-sex  (girl-boy  or  boy-giil)  families.  In  studying  the  effects  of  birth 
order  on  yeais  of  schooling,  then,  it  is  important  to  ascertain  not  only  a 
person's  birth  position  but  also  the  sex  of  the  child  next  to  him. 

Early  talkers  v.  late.  Analyzing  the  records  of  men  in  the  Guidance 
Study,  Dr,  Kenwood  Bartelme  finds  that  the  early  talkers  and  the  late 
show  significant  personality  difference  both  as  children  and  as  adults.  The 
same  findings  seem  to  apply  to  women,  too,  but  in  a  less  clear-cut  fashion. 

The  eaily  talkers  were  taken  to  be  those  who  had  said  at  least  five 
words  befoie  they  were  12  months  old;  the  late  talkers,  those  who  had 
not  done  so  until  after  15  months.  The  boys  in  this  second  group  talked 
well  enough  once  they  got  started,  but  they  had  a  different  peisonality 
style. 

Thiough  adolescence,  at  least,  the  early  talkers  were  on  the  restrained 
and  somber  side.  Their  IQ's  were  consistently  higher  than  those  of  the 
later  talkers  but  largely  because  of  the  difference  in  scores  on  the  verbal 
factor  tests.  In  high  school,  the  early  talkers  were  known  as  eggheads;  at 
30,  they  still  valued  intellectual  matters  and  were  inclined  to  intellectual- 
ize  about  a  subject,  even  to  the  point  of  splitting  hairs,  They  were  also  at 
30  more  practical,  prudent,  and  conservative.  The  late  talkers  were  active, 
lelativcly  uninhibited,  and  even  rebellious.  "The  late-talker,"  Bartelme 
remarks,  "is  the  social  nonconformist." 

The  middle  gioup,  who  began  talking  between  12  and  15  months, 
turned  out  to  be  the  most  conventional. 

Why  one  person  starts  talking  exceptionally  early  and  another  ex- 
ceptionally late,  even  in  the  same  family,  is  not  known,  but  Bartelme  has 
found  one  environmental  difference.  The  early  talker  had  received  more 
than  the  usual  amount  of  attention  from  his  parents,  particularly  from  Iiis 
mother;  the  mother,  in  fact,  had  seemed  to  be  more  involved  with  him 
during  infancy  than  with  her  husband. 

Predicting  Adult  Psychological  Health 

When  the  subjects  of  one  of  the  longitudinal  studies  (Oakland  Growth) 
were  36,  Stewart  divided  a  sample  of  them  into  three  groups: 

1.  Those  with  psychosomatic  disordeis.  Of  the  20  afflicted  persons  in 
this  group,  most  had  either  stomach  ulcer  or  hypertension.  The  others 
suffered  from  migraine  headaches,  spastic  colitis,  asthma,  or  arthritis. 

2.  Those  with  behavioral  maladjustments.  Two  of  the  21  persons  here 
were  alcoholic,  six  had  had  repeated  divorces,  another  six  had  failed  to 
make  a  satisfactory  social  adjustment,  and  seven  had  been  treated  for 
mental  illness. 

143 


lay.  On  tlie  other  hand,  the  late  maturer  was  typie.ifJy 
early  talker,  an  introverted  fellow  who  was  apparently  more  fiiscm 
from  the  start  by  thinking  processes  than  by  action. 

At  30,  the  early  maturei  was  likely  to  have  advanced  farther  in  iiis  u 
than  the  late  matuier,  more  likely  to  be  manied,  and  likely  to  \M\\C 
children. 

Findings  by  Dr,  Maiy  Cover  Jones,  Piofessor  of  Education, 
from  her  study  of  a  different  sample-early  and  late  matming  boys  rii  tie 
Oakland  Growth  Study -con  firm  those  icsults,  add  some  intcicstiitfidfr 
tails,  and  cairy  the  comparison  a  little  fmther  along.  At  33,  the  men  wlw 
had  matured  early  lated  significantly  higher  in  both  sociability  ami  re- 
sponsibility. They  weie  also  more  conventional  in   theii  attitudes  and 
thinking.  Five  years  later  the  differences  were  less  marked,  but  rlic  man 
who  had  matured  eaily  still  appealed  to  be  more  assured  and  somewtal 
less  feaiful,  and  also  less  insightful  and  independent.  More  of  the  c;irl> 
maturing  men  have  attained  executive,  status-conferring  vocational  gojfc 
Tlie  boy  who  most  rapidly  approaches  physical  manhood,  Jones  sug- 
gests, is  the  one  who  is  fust  recognized  by  the  adult  community  iind  wlio 
therefore  is  most  likely  to  take  on-if  he  doesn't  have  them  alrcndy-lfa 
peisonality  tiaits  hkely  to  be  most  valued  by  that  community.  Dr.  IliirH'S 
Peskin,  a  psychologist,  thinks  the  diffeience  found  in  both  studies  ma> 
well  have  a  deep  psychological  basis.  The  early  maturer,  he  suggests*  is!c» 
prepaied  for  the  changes  of  adolescence.  So  he  may  experience  them  a* 
less  tolerable  and  theiefore  less  acceptable.  He  flees,  thetefore,  into  adult 
hood  and  makes  an  early  and  rewarding  commitment  to  the  values  of  Im 
culture.  So  lie  is  "natuially"  more  sociable  and  conforming.  On  I  he  oilier 
hand,  the  late  maturer,  not  having  to  deal  with  the  hormonal-inspired 
drives  till  later,  has  more  time  to  look  around,  expand  his  skills,  and 
develop  a  variety  of  psychological  mechanisms  for  regulating  discs.  Su 
when  pubeiry  comes,  he  tolerates  it  better  and  has  less  need  of  outside 
supports  and  rewards.  This  would  explain  his  greater  insightfuiness. 

Here  again  an  apparent  sex  difference  has  been  found.  Girls  wlio 
reached  matmity  early,  MacFailane  reports,  were  usually  less  confident  as 
adults.  In  school  they  seem  to  have  felt  out  of  things.  These  gj'ils  arc 
described  by  Mai  y  Jones,  on  the  basis  of  observational  ratings,  as  "socially 
disadvantagcd."  However,  their  responses  on  the  Thematic  Apperception 
Test  and  their  self-ieport  scores  indicate  adequate  self -concepts. 

Dr.  Louis  Stewart  adds  tms  finding.  Among  males,  at  least,  the  first- 
born tends  to  mature  earlier  than  an  only  child  or  a  lastborn  child.  This 
psychologist  thinks  the  earlier  maturation  is  somehow  associated  with  the 
events  attendant  on  the  arrival  of  a  new  baby,  for  when  a  mother  was 
pregnant  with  her  second  child,  and  for  a  year  or  two  after  its  birth,  the 
firstborn  showed  an  unusual  spurt  in  growth.  Numerous  experiments  with 
animals  and  some  studies  of  people  show  that  stimulation  in  infancy,  even 
painful  stimulation,  makes  for  growth,  and  separation  from  the  mother 
appears  to  be  an  important  form  of  such  stimulation.  The  mother's  preg- 
nancy and  the  arrival  of  a  new  child,  the  investigator  suggests,  constitute 
painful,  development-spurring  stimulation  for  the  firstborn, 

142 


Incidentally,  othei  work  on  birth  order  by  Dr.  William  T.  Smelser,  also 
a  psychologist,  throws  new  light  on  the  recurrent  finding  that  firstborn 
childien  get  more  education  than  those  born  last.  In  two-child  families 
wheie  both  children  are  of  the  same  sex,  Smelser  finds,  there  is  no  signifi- 
cant difference  in  the  number  of  years  they  go  to  school.  But  where  one 
child  is  a  boy  and  the  other  is  a  girl,  the  firstborn,  of  whichever  sex,  goes 
to  school  significantly  longer.  And  there  is  a  greater  pioportion  of  these 
cross-sex  (girl-boy  or  boy-girl)  families.  In  studying  the  effects  of  birth 
order  on  years  of  schooling,  then,  it  is  important  to  ascertain  not  only  a 
person's  birth  position  but  also  the  sex  of  the  child  next  to  him. 

Early  talkers  v.  late.  Analyzing  the  records  of  men  in  the  Guidance 
Study,  Di.  Kenwood  Baitelme  finds  that  the  early  talkers  and  the  late 
show  significant  personality  difference  both  as  childien  and  as  adults.  The 
lame  findings  seem  to  apply  to  women,  too,  but  in  a  less  clear-cut  fashion. 
The  early  talkers  were  taken  to  be  those  who  had  said  at  least  five 
vords  before  they  were  12  months  old;  the  late  talkers,  those  who  had 
lot  done  so  until  after  15  months.  The  boys  in  this  second  group  talked 
veil  enough  once  they  got  started,  but  they  had  a  different  personality 
tyle. 

Through  adolescence,  at  least,  the  early  talkers  were  on  the  restrained 
nd  somber  side.  Their  IQ's  were  consistently  higher  than  those  of  the 
iter  talkers  but  largely  because  of  the  diffeience  in  scores  on  the  verbal 
ictor  tests.  In  high  school,  the  early  talkers  were  known  as  eggheads;  at 
0,  they  still  valued  intellectual  matters  and  were  inclined  to  intellectual- 
;e  about  a  subject,  even  to  the  point  of  splitting  hairs,  They  were  also  at 
0  more  piactical,  piudent,  and  conservative.  The  late  talkers  were  active, 
slativcjy  uninhibited,  and  even  rebellious.  "The  late-talker,"  Bartelme 
'marks,  "is  the  social  nonconformist." 

The  middle  group,  who  began  talking  between  12  and  15  months, 
irned  out  to  be  the  most  conventional. 

Why  one  person  starts  talking  exceptionally  early  and  another  ex- 
'Ptionally  late,  even  in  the  same  family,  is  not  known,  but  Bartelme  has 
und  one  environmental  difference.  The  early  talker  had  received  moie 
an  the  usual  amount  of  attention  from  his  parents,  particularly  from  his 
other;  the  mother,  in  fact,  had  seemed  to  be  more  involved  with  him 
inng  infancy  than  with  her  husband. 

adicting  Adult  Psychological  Health 

When  the  subjects  of  one  of  the  longitudinal  studies  (Oakland  Growth) 
re  36,  Stewart  divided  a  sample  of  them  into  three  groups: 

1 .  Those  with  psychosomatic  disorders.  Of  the  20  afflicted  persons  in 
s  group,  most  had  either  stomach  ulcer  or  hypertension.  The  others 
Tered  from  migraine  headaches,  spastic  colitis,  asthma,  or  arthritis. 

2.  Those  with  behavioral  maladjustments.  Two  of  the  21  persons  here 
re  alcoholic,  six  had  had  repeated  divorces,  another  six  had  failed  to 
ke   a  satisfactory  social  adjustment,  and  seven  had  been  treated  for 
ntal  illness. 

143 


3.  Those  who  weie  symptom-free:  25. 

Then  he  went  back  to  the  data  showing  the  social  and  emotional  adjust 
ments  of  these  individuals  when  they  had  been  adolescents.  (The  ttoh 
came  from  the  University  of  California  Social  and  Adjustment  Invcntoiy. 
which  had  been  administered  each  year  between  11  and  17.)  He  found 
some  important  differences. 

The  people  with  psychosomatic  ailments  as  adults  weie  reporting  a 
poorer-than-average  adjustment  to  life  when  they  were  only  1 1  years  oil 
which  was  from  15  to  20  years  before  the  diagnosis  of  the  illness.  Oi 
scales  measuring  such  characteristics  as  attitudes  toward  family,  feeling 
about  their  own  woith,  and  ability  to  get  along  with  other  people,  tlws< 
persons  had  rated  themselves  toward  the  low  end.  So  had  the  individuals 
with  behavioral  maladjustments  as  adults.  During  late  adolescence,  how- 
ever, the  scores  on  family  and  social  adjustment  had  improved  among  Hi; 
psychosomatic  group  but  not  among  the  behavioral  maladjustment  group 
During  the  same  period,  those  who  were  later  to  be  afflicted  with  a 
psychosomatic  disorder  also  expressed  a  number  of  vague  physical  com- 
plaints. 

Members  of  the  psychosomatic  group  had  been  marked,  too,  by  UP. 
underlying  tendency  toward  depression,  as  indicated  by  feelings  of  worth- 
lessness,  lack  of  energy,  sleep  disturbance,  and  the  loss-actual  or  feiircd 
of  parental  attention  and  love.  On  all  such  traits  there  had  been  timing 
adolescence  highly  significant  differences  between  the  psycliosomalic 
group  and  the  normal.  The  group  with  behavioral  maladjustments  InJ 
fallen  in  between.  The  results  suggest  to  Stewart  that  a  basic  clcpicssiw 
tendency  is  an  important  factor  in  the  onset  of  psychosomatic  disorder. 
And  he  is  inclined  to  agree  with  some  other  investigators  that  virtually  a'l 
illness  is  caused  by  psychic  as  well  as  somatic  factors, 

To  try  to  find  the  basis  of  the  maladjustments  noted  during  udolev 
cence,  Stewart  is  now  analyzing  the  records  of  another  study  (GuklaiiH) 
which  go  back  to  infancy.  His  preliminary  findings  confirm  tluil  lululli 
with  psychosomatic  ailments  or  with  psychological  problems  worse  \\m 
usual  had  been  poorly  adjusted  adolescents.  Further,  the  new  fimliii£i 
indicate  that  (1)  members  of  both  groups  came  from  families  whcic.vti) 
early  in  the  children's  life,  there  had  been  more  disturbance,  and  IM 
satisfaction  and  security,  than  usual,  (2)  members  of  both  groups  tended 
to  be  those  who  had  matured  either  very  early  or  very  late, 

This  second  finding  does  not  imply  that  early  and  late  ma  Hirers  aie 
inevitably  bound  for  trouble.  The  processes  associated  with  either  cxlrem* 
of  the  maturation  rate  do  seem  to  produce  not  only  differences  in  person- 
ality, as  noted  earlier,  but  also  a  higher  than  average  potential  for  illiiM 
and  psychological  difficulties.  Stewart  is  now  trying  to  find  curly  famib 
and  childhood  patterns  that  distinguish  the  two  groups-thc  ill  iuid  ifo 
maladjusted.  He  is  also  looking  for  childhood  factors  that  distinpMi 
people  with  one  type  of  ailment  from  those  with  another. 

In  related  work,  Drs.  Norman  Livson  and  Harvey  Peskin  have  ten 
trying  to  determine  which,  if  any,  specific  characteristics  of  a  child,  dis- 
played at  which  particular  age,  can  be  used  to  predict  his  psychological 

144 


health  as  an  adult.  The  subjects  were  64  young  adults,  from  the  Guidance 
Study,  who  had  been  rated  for  psychological  health  at  30  by  comparing 
their  scores  on  a  personality  appraisal  with  theoretically  ideal  scores.  (The 
ideal  scores  were  a  composite  of  those  made  by  foui  clinical  psychologists 
as  they  attempted  to  define  a  fully  healthy  peison.)  The  adults'  ratings 
were  then  compared  with  their  ratings  as  children,  from  the  ages  of  five 
through  16,  on  numerous  behavior  and  personality  scales. 

High  scores  on  certain  characteristics  during  the  years  from  11  to  13, 
but  only  during  those  years,  were  found  to  be  significantly  related  to 
adult  psychological  health.  The  healthiest  men  were  those  who  as  boys  of 
11-13  had  been  lelatively  extroverted,  cheerful,  telaxed,  and  expressive, 
and  relatively  immune  to  irritability.  The  women  had  been  relatively  inde- 
pendent, confident,  and  inquiring— and  had  shown  a  hearty  attitude  to- 
ward food. 

The  11-13  age  period  proved  significant,  the  investigators  speculate, 
because  it  encompassed  the  transitional  period  from  elementary  school  to 
junior  high.  Now  once  again,  as  when  he  had  left  the  family  to  enter 
school,  the  child  had  to  take  an  important  step  toward  maturity.  "The 
demands  and  opportunities  of  the  junior  high  school,  for  both  boys  and 
girls,"  the  investigators  suggest,  "may  represent  so  profound  a  difference 
from  elementary  school  as  to  constitute  a  qualitatively  new  experience. 
The  manner  in  which  the  child  responds  to  the  transition-actively  inviting 
or  passively  withdrawing  from  the  new  experience-tells  us  something 
about  how  healthy  an  adult  he  will  be." 

The  psychological  health  of  these  subjects  will  be  assessed  again  during 
the  40-year  follow-up.  The  investigators  plan  also  to  look  for  factors  in 
the  family  environment  during  childhood  that  may  portend  good  or  poor 
psychological  health  in  latei  life. 


Children  Who  Turned  Out  Better  or  Worse  Than  Expected 

Because  of  the  work  reported  in  the  preceding  section  and  of  the  re- 
search under  way  at  Berkeley  and  other  centers,  the  investigators  think  we 
shall  be  better  able  to  predict  while  a  person  is  still  in  school  the  probable 
state  of  his  mental  health  as  an  adult -and,  if  the  outlook  is  poor,  to  take 
steps  to  alter  it.  They  emphasize,  however,  that  the  relationships  reported 
are  based  on  group  averages  and  that  in  every  group  studied  there  were 
individuals  who  did  not  conform. 

More  than  20  years  ago,  when  the  children  in  the  Guidance  Study  were 
IS,  MacFarlane  and  her  associates  made  predictions  about  them  as  adults 
—their  personalities,  their  success  in  marriage  and  work,  their  ability  to 
cope  with  the  problems  of  life:  in  short,  their  mental  health.  Though  the 
investigators  had  had  few  scientific  guides,  they  were  surprised  by  the 
results  of  the  analysis  after  the  subjects  were  followed  up  at  the  age  of  30. 
In  many  cases,  the  predictions  turned  out  to  have  been  wrong.  The  rea- 
sons ought  to  be  helpful  for  parents,  teachers,  doctors,  and  everyone  else 
associated  with  children. 

145 


many  of  the  most  matuie  adults-integrated,  competent,  clear  about 
their  values,  and  accepting  of  themselves  and  others-were  found  tolumr 
been  those  who  as  youngsteis  had  been  faced  with  difficult  situations  and 
whose  characteristic  lesponses  had  seemed  to  compound  their  problems. 
They  included  chronic  rebels  who  had  been  expelled  from  school,  Mac- 
Failane  reports,  highly  intelligent  students  who  were  nevertheless  academic 
failures,  children  filled  with  hostility,  and  unhappy,  withdrawn  schizoids. 
But  the  behavior  regaided  by  the  investigatois  as  disiuptive  to  growth  and 
matuiity  seemed  in  these  cases  to  have  led  directly  or  indirectly  to  adult 
strength.  One  of  the  foinier  rebels  recalled  that  he  had  desperately  needed 
approval  "even  if  it  was  from  kids  as  maladjusted  as  I  was."  To  maintain  his 
rebel  status,  he  said,  he  had  had  to  commit  all  of  his  intelligence  and 
stamina,  a  circumstance  he  believed  had  contributed  to  his  adult  strength 
in  tackling  difficult  problems.  "I  hope  my  children  find  less  wasteful  ways 
to  mature,"  he  remaiked,  "-but  who  knows?" 

Close  to  halt  of  the  subjects  fell  into  the  gioup  for  whom  crippled  or 
inadequate  personalities  had  been  predicted.  But  as  adults  almost  all  of 
them  were  bettei  than  had  been  expected,  and  some  of  them  far  better. 

One  man,  for  example,  held  back  three  times  in  elementary  school,  had 
not  graduated  from  liigh  school  until  he  was  21 .  His  IQ  over  the  years  had 
aveiaged  less  than  100.  He  had  shown  little  inteicst  in  studies,  school 
activities,  or  people.  The  school  had  not  recommended  that  he  go  to 
college.  The  staff  thought  he'd  always  be  a  misfit,  a  sidelines.  But  1 2  years 
later  he  was  a  talented  environmental  designer,  a  good  father,  and  an 
active  worker  in  community  affairs.  "Obviously,"  says  MacFarlane,  "his 
tested  IQ's  were  no  measuie  of  his  true  ability." 

One  girl,  who  early  was  suspicious  of  and  even  hostile  to  members  of 
the  study  staff,  lived  with  a  rejecting  mothei  and  a  poorly  adjusted  aunt. 
She  hated  home  and  she  hated  school,  paitly  because  of  her  poor  clothes. 
To  escape,  she  married  while  still  in  high  school  a  boy  as  erratic  mid 
immature  as  she.  They  soon  separated.  At  30,  with  the  investigators 
dreading  the  impending  interview,  in  came  a  personable,  well-groomed, 
gracious  woman  with  two  buoyant  but  well-mannered  childien.  She  had 
married  again  and  was  living  a  stable,  contented  life. 
Why  were  the  predictions  wrong  in  such  cases? 

For  one  thing,  MacFarlane  answers,  the  investigatois  gave  too  much 
weight  to  the  troublesome  and  pathogenic  elements  in  a  child's  life-quite 
naturally,  in  view  of  the  studies  that  have  traced  neuroses  and  psychoses 
to  such  elements-and  too  little  weight  to  the  healthful,  maturity-inducing 
elements.  (The  lattei  were  present  even  in  the  case  of  the  girl  who  sought 
escape  through  marriage  at  17.  She  always  remembered  that  another  aunt 
had  given  her  affection  and  happiness-had  heloed  her  wlant  seeds  that 


end.  As  an  example,  MacFarlane  cites  hurt  feelings,  m  both  boys  and  girls, 
as  "a  veiy  successful  parent-manipulation  tool."  In  changed  situations, 
such  early  useful  devices  lost  their  effectiveness,  and  the  big  majority  of 
the  young  people  then  dropped  or  modified  them,  sometimes  not  without 
difficulty.  With  a  number  of  girls,  the  game  of  getting  their  feelings  hurt 
was  can  led  over  into  marriage. 

Sometimes  the  undesirable  but  long-continued  patterns  were  converted, 
to  the  investigators'  surprise,  into  almost  the  opposite  characteristics.  For 
example,  it  was  predicted  that  overdependent  boys  with  energetic  and 
dominant  mothers  would  pick  wives  like  the  mothers  and  continue  the 
pattern  of  overdependencc.  Instead,  nearly  all  such  boys  chose  girls  who 
were  lacking  in  confidence.  The  boys  thus  won  themselves  a  role  as  the 
proud  male  protector  and  giver  of  support,  and  in  this  role,  says  the 
investigator,  they  thrived. 

Along  the  same  line,  a  number  of  those  in  the  study  who  were  socially 
inept  and  insecure  as  children  and  adolescents  became,  again  to  everyone's 
surprise,  highly  successful  salesmen.  Looking  back,  MacFarlane  sees  this  as 
a  quite  natural  transformation.  The  boys  did  not  have  easy,  intimate 
relationships  growing  up  because  they  did  not  have  them  at  home.  As 
adults  they  still  fear  intimate  relationships  but  have  an  unconscious  desire 
for  social  intercourse.  Selling  gives  them  the  needed  contacts  without  the 
feared  intimacy.  (She  could  be  wrong,  she  adds.  The  director  of  sales 
training  for  a  large  firm  told  her  he  deliberately  picked  shy  people,  be- 
cause they  would  concentrate  on  selling  the  product  instead  of  them- 
selves.) 

One  man,  who  is  remembered  with  special  pride,  is  the  highly  successful 
manager  of  a  large  business  concern.  Years  ago  he  had  been  a  shy  little 
boy  without  friends.  Though' he  had  dropped  in  from  time  to  time  to  see 
members  of  the  Guidance  Study  Staff,  his  communications  had  often 
been  limited  to  hello  and  goodbye.  After  high  school  he  enlisted  and, 
since  he  had  taken  some  shop  courses,  he  was  asked  to  help  with  the 
building  and  repair  work  at  his  Army  post.  First  he  was  flattered  that 
anyone  should  think  he  could  do  any  tiling;  then  he  was  proud  that  he 
could  actually  do  it.  After  his  service,  he  went  to  business  school,  where 
he  got  all  A's,  as  compared  to  C's  in  high  school.  Now  he  says  the  most 
interesting  part  of  his  job  is  to  give  people  "something  to  do  that  is  a  little 
harder  than  what  they  have  done  or  think  they  can  do-but  not  something 
they  would  fail  at-and  then  to  watch  them  expand.  Nothing  is  more 
exciting  to  me  than  to  see  people  get  confidence"-which  he  himself  had 
lacked  for  so  long. 

A  number  of  other  subjects  had  had  similar  experiences.  They  did  not 
achieve  "ego  identity"-did  not  find  themselves-until  they  had  been 
forced  into  or  been  given  an  opportunity  to  take  on  a  responsible  role  that 
gave  them  the  sense  of  worth  they  had  missed  at  home.  Often  these 
people  did  not  find  this  new  and  satisfying  role  until  they  had  left  both 
their  childhood  homes  and  their  home  towns. 

147 


"Don't  give  up  on  our  present  generation  of  adolescents,"  MacFarlane 
urges.  "Many  of  ours  came  through  bad  times  and  developed  into  mature, 
stable  adults  in  spite  of  our  fears." 

In  a  speech  before  The  American  Academy  of  Pediatrics  recently,  (lie 
investigator  quoted  comments  made  spontaneously  by  a  number  of  the 
Guidance  Study  subjects  at  30.  Some  examples: 

"When  I  was  confused  and  worried,  the  Institute  was  the  only  place  I  could  talk  ant 
loud  to  myself  and  find  out  what  I  thought  and  felt." 

"I  sensed  your  respect  for  me,  even  when  I  knew  I  wasn't  acting  very  sensibly  and 
knew  you  wouldn't  have  had  respect  for  me  if  there  wasn't  something  there  lo  respect 
because,  beheve  me,  I  can  tell  a  phony  in  a  split  second-because  at  times  I'm  n  plumy 
myself." 

"You  asked  questions,  you  listened,  but  you  were  the  only  grown-ups  who  didn't 
give  advice.  You  helped  me  to  ferret  things  out  for  myself,  to  make  my  own  decisions. 
I  try  to  carry  this  on  in  the  raising  of  my  children." 

The  investigator  then  pled  with  the  doctors  to  take  a  similar  role  wilh 
their  patients.  "You  pediatricians  are  the  only  professional  group,"  she 
said,  "that  can  furnish  continuity  and  interest  over  the  long  age-span  of 
growth  from  babyhood  to  maturity,  provided,  of  course,  you  have  Ik 
temperament  to  be  sympathetically  interested  in  the  vagaries  of  Hie 
human  struggle  for  competence  and  maturity.  Provided,  too,  you  can 
accept  the  fact  that  you  can't  play  God  or  believe  you  know  nil  the 
answers,  because  one  tiling  we  have  learned  is  how  little  we  know.  Pro- 
vided, too,  that  you  can  train  yourselves  to  look  for  strengths  in  indi- 
viduals and  their  situations  and  not  just  for  pathology.  If  you  don't  fur- 
nish this  function,  what  professional  will?  If  teaching  departments  don't 
incite  interest,  who  will?" 

About  20  percent  of  the  cases  turned  out  worse  than  expected.  These 
included  many  of  the  persons  who  as  children  and  adolescents  had  had 
easy,  confidence-inducing  lives,  free  of  severe  strains  and  marked  by 
academic,  social,  or  athletic  success.  Prominent  among  these  were  a  high 
proportion  of  the  men  who  had  been  outstanding  athletes  in  high  school 
and  of  the  women  who  as  girls  had  been  pretty  and  exceptionally  popular 
At  30,  many  of  these  people  had  failed  to  live  up  to  their  potentialities 
and  were  puzzled  and  discontented. 

MacFarlane  gives  several  possible  explanations.  Early  success  may  have 
led  to  unreal  expectations  and  to  a  draining  of  energies  into  maintaining   • 
an  image.  It  may  have  sidetracked  the  development  of  patterns  ami  atti- 
tudes that  would  have  made  adult  life  more  rewarding.  Perhaps  there 
't  enough  stress  in  these  youngsters'  lives  to  foster  development.  She 
some  of  the  people  in  this  group  will  work  free  from  their  tails- 
's  as  young  adults  and  will  yet  live  up  to  their  predictions  for  them;  ' 
«>"  go  through  live  wondering  what  happened  and  where  they 

people  turned  out  as  predicted .  Among  Ihe 
rcontrolled,  who  had  built  a  psycho- 

-~eiw..  «^iW  agauui  me  uangers  10  be  found  in  other  people  and  in  llfcia  - 
general.  As  young  adults  they  still  had  the  shield.  It  seems  to  have  pro- 
tected them,  MacFarlane  reports,  but  it  also-by  denying  them  access  to  ; 

148 


many  kinds  of  learning  experience-has  impoverished  them.  The  second 
main  group  includes  the  youngsters  who  had  been  subjected  to  marked 
variability  in  family  treatment,  being  handled  indulgently  one  day  and 
slapped  down  the  next.  Neither  as  adolescents  nor  young  adults  had  they 
developed  stable  patterns  of  behavior.  Of  the  nine  adults  in  this  study  who 
were  found  to  be  compulsive  drinkers  at  the  age  of  30,  all  but  one  came 
from  this  group.  Perhaps  significantly,  the  compulsive  drinkers  had  mani- 
fested physical  vulnerability,  too-acute  allergies,  beginning  in  infancy. 

The  Effects  of  Guidance 

The  patents  of  half  of  the  children  in  the  Guidance  Study  were  en- 
couraged to  turn  to  the  staff  psychologists  for  discussion  of  problems 
whenever  they  wished.  Many  did  so.  Through  these  discussions,  they  were 
helped  to  a  better  understanding  not  only  of  their  children's  behaviors  and 
attitudes  but  also  of  their  own  and  their  spouses'.  The  control  families 
were  not  intensively  interviewed  and  discussions  were  avoided  or  kept  to  a 
minimum.  The  subjects  in  the  guidance  and  control  groups-the  children 
who  have  grown  up  and  become  parents  themselves— are  now  being  com- 
pared in  order  to  help  explain  why  they  grew  up  to  be  the  kind  of  parents 
they  are  and  have  the  kind  of  children  they  have. 

So  far,  only  hard  fact  has  emerged:  The  group  whose  parents  received 
little  or  no  opportunity  for  discussion  of  interpersonal  relations  and  chil- 
dren's problems  has  had  about  four  times  as  many  divorces  as  the  guid- 
ance group.  Dr.  Ann  Stout,  the  psychologist  who  is  handling  the  parent- 
child  study,  finds  other  evidence  pointing  to  the  conclusion  that  when 
parents  are  encouraged  to  discuss  family  problems  with  professional 
workers,  their  children  tend  as  adults  to  have  more  flexible  qualities  and  a 
better  ability  to  cope  with  situations.  As  she  puts  it,  the  guidance  seems  to 
have  tempered  some  of  the  negative  factors  influencing  the  children's 
development.  For  example,  there  are  persons  in  both  groups  who  were 
ebullient  as  youngsters  but  who  for  some  reason,  at  some  point,  took  on  a 
ratlier  depressive  attitude.  They  are  not  getting  the  satisfaction  out  of  life 
that  they  should.  However,  the  depressive  subjects  in  the  guidance  group 
show  more  resilience  than  those  in  the  control  group,  and  a  tendency  to 
find  more  satisfaction. 

Antecedents  of  Smoking 

In  1  964  the  report  to  the  United  States  Surgeon  General  on  "Smoking 
and  Health"  stated  that  "While  rebellion  may  play  a  role  in  the  initiation 
of  smoking,  perhaps  an  important  one,  there  is  not  much  evidence  for  it. 
Claims  in  the  literature  are  at  best  based  on  circumstantial  suggestive 
evidence,  linked  to  conclusions  by  a  chain  of  questionable  assumptions." 
Since  then,  Stewart  and  Livson,  using  data  from  the  Guidance  and  Oak- 
land Growth  Studies,  have  turned  up  evidence  that  cigarette  smokers 
actually  are  more  rebellious  than  nonsmokers  and  that  this  greater  degree 
of  rebelliousness  is  part  of  the  smoker's  personality  long  before  he  starts 

149 


smoking.  It  appears  even  during  the  earliest  years  of  school.  In  a  separate 
study  using  a  different  methodology,  Clausen  finds  that  the  youngsters 
who  became  smokers  tended  to  be  less  controlled  and  more  aggressive 
than  the  otheis  and  to  differ  also  in  other  important  traits  and  in  their 
backgrounds.  The  nonsmoking  boys  had  a  stronger  drive  to  get  ahead,  and 
they  still  have  it  as  adults. 

To  measure  rebelliousness,  Stewart  and  Livson  used  the  teachers' 
ratings  on  the  children's  behavior  in  school  and  on  their  attitude  toward 
school  and,  where  the  ratings  were  not  available,  grades  on  conduct.  A 
"resistance  to  authority"  measure,  taken  during  four  years  of  high  school, 
was  also  available  for  those  subjects  from  the  Oakland  Growth  Study.  This 
was  the  average  of  independent  ratings  made  by  three  staff  membeis  after 
observing  the  students  in  a  variety  of  social  situations. 

From  ages  five  to  15,  the  persons  who  later  became  smokers  were 
found  to  show  more  evidence  of  rebellious  attitudes.  For  all  the  subjects 
studied,  the  diffeicnce  was  clearest  during  the  sixth  and  seventh  grades, 
when  it  was  statistically  significant  for  each  of  the  two  groups  and  for 
both  girls  and  boys.  For  those  subjects  related  on  "resistance  to  author- 
ity," the  difference  continued  to  be  significant  through  high  school. 

As  adults,  too,  the  smokeis— who  comprised  about  55  percent  of  the 
160  subjects  studied  during  their  early  30's~were  rated  more  rebellious. 
The  measure  this  time  was  a  scale  intended  to  show  the  degree  to  which  a 
person  conforms  to  the  mores  of  our  society.  (Bankers  have  been  found  to 
rate  at  the  top  of  this  measure,  which  is  socialization  scale  of  the  Cali- 
fornia Psychological  Inventory;  juvenile  delinquents  and  criminals  rate  at 
the  bottom.)  The  nonsmokers  scored  significantly  highei  than  the 
smokers:  the  finding  could  have  occurred  by  chance  one  time  in  100  in 
the  case  of  women,  and  one  time  in  1 000  in  the  case  of  the  men. 

So  what?  The  investigators  respond  that  smoking  and  adolescent  rebel- 
lion against  authority  have  long  been  linked  in  popular  thought  and  that 
this  study  provides  evidence  they  actually  are  linked.  If  this  is  so,  the 
investigators  continue,  an  antismoking  campaign  based  upon  authoritative 
pronouncements  has  little  chance  of  success:  a  more  subtle  strategy  is 
required. 

Stewart  and  Livson  refer  to  a  handful  of  recent  studies  that  have  used  a 
measure  (the  Psychopathic  Deviate  Scale  of  the  Minnesota  Multiphasic 
Personality  Inventory)  very  similar  to  the  one  employed  in  this  investiga- 
tion to  rate  rebelliousness  in  adults.  These  studies  have  found  significantly 
higher  scores  among  (a)  people  addicted  either  to  alcohol  or  narcotics,  (b) 
smokers  in  a  psychiatric  outpatient  population,  and  (c)  smokers  in  two 
samples  of  male  college  students.  The  Berkeley  investigators  suggest  that 
smoking  is  an  addiction,  sharing  with  the  other  addictions  a  common 
origin  in  some  underlying  resentment  of  authority  and  being,  like  the 
other  addictions,  not  an  isolated  habit  but  an  expression  of  "pervasive 

nts  of  smoking,  Clausen  sent  a 
subjects  in  the  Oakland  Growth 
received  answers  from  1 23,  and 


then  searched  for  personality  and  background  factors-asrecoided  during 
adolescence— that  might  differentiate  between  those  who  took  up  smoking 
and  those  who  did  not,  and  between  heavy  and  light  smokers. 

Perhaps  the  clearest  finding,  Clausen  reports,  is  that  the  adolescents 
who  did  not  become  smokers  were  more  controlled  in  high  school,  more 
oriented  to  adult  values,  relatively  unaggressive,  and  modest  in  tncir  views 
of  themselves.  The  boys  were  seen  as  well-adjusted  and  creative  and  as 
having  a  strong  desire  to  achieve;  in  senior  high  they  enjoyed  piestige  and 
popularity.  The  girls  who  did  not  take  up  smoking  were  rated  as  conven- 
tional, unassuming,  calm,  serious,  and  self-sufficient.  Though  well- 
adjusted,  they  were  less  popular  than  the  girls  who  became  smokers. 

As  a  partial,  quite  incomplete,  explanation  of  these  differences,  Clausen 
finds  that  the  boys  and  girls  who  matured  later  than  the  others  were  more 
likely  to  start  smoking  and  continue.  Though  there  is  no  direct  link  be- 
tween biology  and  smoking,  the  investigator  hypothesizes,  the  person  who 
finds  himself  lagging  behind  the  gang  in  physical  maturity  may  turn  to 
smoking  as  a  sign  that  he,  too,  is  growing  up. 

Clausen  also  finds  differences  in  the  characteristics  of  the  mothers  of 
smokers  and  nonsmokcrs.  The  motheis  of  the  boys  who  remained  non- 
smokers  had  been  rated  as  effective,  nonneurotic  women— significantly 
less  talkative  and  more  clearheaded  and  cheerful  than  the  mothers  of  the 
other  boys.  The  mothers  of  the  girls  who  remained  nonsmokeis  showed  an 
accepting  rather  than  a  critical  attitude. 

The  actual  smoking  habits  of  the  parents  strongly  influenced  those  of 
the  girls,  but  not  of  the  boys.  Among  those  girls  with  at  least  one  parent 
who  smoked,  a  fourth  eventually  became  very  heavy  smokers  and  only  a 
fifth  did  not  smoke  at  all.  When  neither  parent  smoked,  scarcely  any  of 
the  girls  became  very  heavy  smokers  and  nearly  half  never  smoked. 

Lack  of  poise  would  be  an  important  antecedent  of  smoking,  Clausen 
had  thought,  but  this  turned  out  to  be  incorrect,  especially  among  the 
girls.  The  girls  who  remained  nonsmokers  were  rated  less  poised  and 
socially  skilled  than  the  others  and  at  the  same  time  more  unaffected  and 
more  composed. 

On  the  basis  of  intensive  interviews,  averaging  12  hours  in  length,  the 
adults  show  many  of  the  same  personality  traits  that  characterized  them 
as  adolescents,  The  nonsmoking  men  still  have  a  stronger  drive  for  achieve- 
ment and  are  more  effective.  They  are  in  tighter  control  of  themselves, 
less  self-indulgent,  and  more  self-satisfied.  Though  the  nonsmokers  were 
rated  significantly  less  assertive  in  adolescence,  as  adults  they  arc  slightly 
more  assertive  than  the  smokers,  Probably  this  change  has  occurred, 
Clausen  thinks,  because  most  of  the  nonsmokers  have  experienced  a  con- 
siderable measure  of  occupational  success.  Moderate  smokers  have  done 
nearly  as  well,  but  heavy  smokers  much  less  well.  Among  the  women, 
nonsmokers  remain  more  conforming.  They  are  also  rated  more  fearful 
and  more  likely  to  manifest  guilt  than  are  women  who  become  smokers. 

The  questionnaire  relating  to  current  smoking  has  now  been  sent  to 
subjects  of  all  these  studies.  A  monograph  reporting  findings  will  deal  not 

151 


only  with  antecedents  and  con  elates  of  smoking  but  also  with  compari- 
sons between  addicted  smokeis  and  those  able  to  give  up  or  cut  down  on 
tobacco  use. 

Antecedents  of  Drinking 

As  anothei  example  of  using  a  longitudinal  study  to  answer  questions 
almost  impossible  to  answer  in  any  other  way,  Mary  Jones  has  been 
studying  the  personalities  of  drinkers  and  nondrinkers.  Her  question  was: 
Do  personality  characteristics  associated  with  a  given  drinking  pattern 
show  up  early  in  life,  before  the  pattein  has  been  established?  The  tenta- 
tive answer  is  that  they  do. 

In  the  work  to  date,  68  men  and  70  women  in  the  Oakland  Growth 
Study  were  classified  according  to  their  drinking  habits  and  theii  reasons 
for  drinking.  The  subjects  were  in  thcii  middle  forties.  Then  their  person- 
alities weie  assessed,  through  the  use  of  California  Q  Set,  for  three  age 
levels:  junior  high  school,  senior  high  school,  and  adulthood. 

More  than  half  of  the  behavioral  items  that  differentiated  problem 
drinkeis  from  modciatc  di inkers  (typically,  a  drink  or  two  before  dinner, 
three  or  four  at  a  party)  and  abstainers  in  adulthood  were  found  to  have 
differentiated  them  also  in  junioi  high  school.  For  example,  the  men 
problem  drinkers,  compaied  with  the  other  men  studied,  were  found  to  be 
rebellious,  self-indulgent,  gregarious,  unpredictable,  and  disorganized. 
They  were  less  dependable,  less  considerate,  less  fastidious,  and  less 
moralistic.  As  junior  high  students,  these  men  had  been  marked  by  the 
same  characteristics.  And  they  had  been  more  concerned  than  the  others 
to  demonstrate  their  masculinity.  Behavior  duiing  the  junior  high  school 
years  proved  to  be  a  better  predictor  of  adult  drinking  patterns  Hum 
behavior  during  later  adolescence. 

On  the  rating  that  distinguished  the  three  groups  both  as  adults  and 
junior  high  students,  the  men  who  weic  problem  drinkers  as  adults  usually 
stood  at  one  end  of  the  scale,  the  abstainers  at  the  other,  and  the 
moderate  drinkers  in  between. 

Preliminary  findings  indicate  that  women  problem  drinkers  resemble 
their  male  counterparts  in  respect  to  instability,  unprcdictableness  anil 
impulsiveness.  However,  they  tend  to  be  introversivc  and  more  marked  by 
feelings  of  depression,  self-doubt,  and  distrust  than  the  men. 

Looking  Ahead 

When  the  longitudinal  studies  began  at  Berkeley,  there  was  little  docu- 
mented knowledge  about  factors  that  helped  shape  intelligence,  person- 
ality, and  mental  health  from  childhood  onward.  Now  there  is  a  good 
deal,  thanks  not  only  to  these  pioneering  investigations  but  also  to  numer- 
ous other  studies  undertaken  more  recently . 

Because  of  man's  complexity  and  the  great  variety  of  the  influences 
pressing  upon  him,  much  remains  to  be  learned -a  statement  that  may 
always  be  true.  But  the  body  of  our  knowledge  is  being  steadily  increased. 

152 


Among  the  continuing  investigations  at  Berkeley,  two  seem  especially 
important:  the  attempt  to  identify  those  factors  in  infancy  and  childhood 
that  predispose  to  psychosomatic  illness  and  psychological  maladjustment, 
and  the  attempt  to  ferret  out  the  influence  of  heredity  on  certain  abilities, 
and  characteristics. 

Other  important  work  under  way  includes  research  on 

o  The  relationship  of  physical  factors,  such  as  body  build,  to  specific 
types  of  intellectual  function,  such  as  mathematical  ability 

o  The  ethical,  religious,  political,  and  other  values  held  by  a  poison's 
family  as  he  grew  up;  the  values  he  look  with  him  into  maniagc;  the 
values  that  now  dominate  his  home. 

«  What  difference  it  makes  if  one  paicnt  is  the  disciplinarian  rather 
than  the  othei,  or  both. 

«  The  value  of  the  Roischaeh  test,  which  was  administered  to  Guidance 
Study  members  for  7  years  during  adolescence  and  again  at  30,  in  predict- 
ing psychological  health. 

»  The  relationship  between  a  person's  interest  and  satisfactions  during 
childhood  to  his  personality  and  psychological  health  as  an  adult. 

Staff  members  are  also  busy  fitting  together  and  interpieting  the  find- 
ings so  that  these  can  be  of  the  widest  use.  Important  books  coming  up  in- 
clude (1 )  "Ways  of  Personality  Development:  Continuity  and  Change  from 
Adolescence  to  Adulthood,"  by  Jack  Block  and  Norma  Haan,  which  uses 
data  from  both  the  Oakland  Growth  and  the  Guidance  studies;  (2)  "Chil- 
dren of  the  Depression,"  an  analysis  by  Dr.  Glen  H.  Elder,  Jr.,  of  the 
immediate  and  long-term  effects  of  the  depression  on  the  subjects  in  the 
Oakland  Growth  Study;  and  (3)  "The  Course  of  Human  Development,"  a 
collection  of  major  papers  from,  and  new  essays  about  three  studies, 
edited  by  Drs.  Mary  Jones,  Bayley,  Honzik,  and  MacFarlane.  Also  Dr. 
Clausen  is  working  on  a  major  "life  careers"  monogiaph  based  on  informa- 
tion from  the  Oakland  Growth  Study.  It  seeks  to  answer:  What  are  the 
major  influences  upon  a  person's  performance  in  the  most  salient  loles  of 
adulthood-thosc  of  worker,  spouse,  parent  and  community  participant? 

In  the  near  future,  Berkeley's  Institute  of  Human  Development  liopes 
to  establish  a  program  of  Intel-generational  Studies  of  Development  and 
Aging  that  will  use  the  people  who  have  been  participating  in  the  present 
sliidics-thc  original  subjects,  their  surviving  parents,  and  the  subjects' 
children.  The  questions  to  be  investigated  include  the  patterns  and 
processes  of  aging,  the  hcritability  of  traits  and  abilities,  and  the  similar- 
ities and  differences  in  family  patterns  and  styles  of  life  from  one  gen- 
eration to  another. 

Research  Grants:  Mil  6238,  8135,  5300 
Date  of  Interviews.   April  1967 


153 


References: 

Bayley,  N.  The  life  span  as  a  frame  of  reference  in  psychological  research.   Vita 

Humana,  6  125,  1963. 
Bayley,  N  Consistency  of  maternal  and  child  behaviors  in  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study. 

Vita  Humana,  7.73,  1964. 
Bayley,  N.  Research  in  child  development'  a  longitudinal  perspective  Merrill  Palmer 

Quarterly  of  Behavior  and  Development,  11'3,  1965. 
Bayley,  N.  Age-trends  in  mental  scores:  ages  16  to  36  yeais.  Paper  for  American 

Psychological  Association,  1966. 
Bayley,  N.  Learning  in  adulthood:  the  role  of  intelligence.  In.  Analyses  of  Concept 

Learning.  New  York1  Academic  Press,  1966. 
Bayley,  N,  Cognition.  Paper  for  University  of  West  Virginia  Conference  on  Theory  ami 

Methods  of  Research  and  Aging,  1967. 
Bayley,   N.    Behavioral   correlates  of  mental  growth:   birth  to  36   years.  Paper  foi 

American  Psychological  Association,  1967. 
Bayley,  N.  and  Schaefer,  E.  S.  Correlations  of  mateinal  and  child  behaviors  with  the 

development  of  mental  abilities:   data  from  the  Berkeley  Growth  Study.  Mono- 
graphs of  the  Society  for  Research  in  Child  Development,  29(6).  1-80,  1 964, 
Branson,  W.  C.  Early  antecedents  of  emotional  expressiveness  and  reactivity  control. 

Child  Development,  37:793-810,  1966. 
Branson,  W,  C.  Central  orientations:  a  study  of  behavior  organization  from  childhood 

to  adolescence.  Child  Development,  37' 1,  1966. 
Cameron,  J.;  Livson,  N.;  and  Bayley,  N.  Infant  vocalizations  and  then  relationship  to 

mature  intelligence.  Science,  157'3786,  1967 
Clausen,  J.   A.  Adolescent  antecedents  of  cigarette  smoking    data  from  Oakland 

Growth  Study.  Social  Science  and  Medicine,  1:4, 1968. 
Haan,  N.  Proposed  model  of  ego  functioning:  coping  and  defense  mechanisms  in 

relationship  to  IQ  change.  Psychological  Monographs   General  and  Applied,  77 '8, 

1963. 
Honzik,  M.  P.  A.  sex  difference  in  the  age  of  onset  of  the  parent-child  resemblance  in 

intelligence.  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology,  54:5,  1963. 
Honzik,  M.  P.  The  environment  and  mental  growth  from  21  months  to  30  yeais.  Paper 

for  International  Congress  of  Psychology,  1966, 
Honxik,  M.  P.  Environmental  correlate's  of  mental  growth:  prediction  from  the  family 

settingat  21  months.  Child  Development,  38:337-363,  1967. 
Jones,  M.  C.  Psychological  correlates  of  somatic  development.  Child  Development, 

36:4,  1965. 
Jones,  M.  C.  Personality  correlates  and  antecedents  of  drinking  patterns  in  adult 

males.  Journal  of  Consulting  and  Clinical  Psychology,  32:1,  1968 
Livson,  N.,  and  Peskm,  H.  The  prediction  of  adult  psychological  health  in  a  longi- 
tudinal study.  Journal  of  Abnormal  Psychology,  72 '509-5 1 8,  1967. 
MacFarlane,  J.  From  infancy  to  adulthood.  Childhood  Education,  39:336-342,  1963. 
MacFarlane,  J.  Perspectives  on  personality  consistency  and  change  ftom  the  Guidance 

Study.  VitaHumana,  7-115,  1964. 
MacFarlane,   J.  The  dilemmas  of  adolescents.  Paper  for  American  Academy   of 

Pediatrics,  1967. 
Peskm,  H.  Puberral  onset  and  ego  functioning:  a  psychoanalytic  approach.  Journal  of 

Abnormal  Psychology,  72:1-15,  1967. 
Stewart,  L.,  and  Livson,  N.  Smoking  and  rebelliousness:  a  longitudinal  study  fiom 

childhood  to  maturity.  Journal  of  Consulting  Psychology,  30:325-329,  1966. 


154 


Investigator: 

John  W.M.  Whiting,  Ph.D. 

Harvard  University 
Cambridge,  Mass, 

Co -contributor: 

Shulamlth  M.  Gunders 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 


John  W.  M.  Whiting  and  follow  workeis  at  Harvard  and  elsewhere  have 
searched  the  world  over  for  moie  information  on  some  of  the  forces  that 
turn  a  child-born  with  the  same  potentialities,  on  the  average,  as  other 
children-into  an  adult  typical  of  the  society  in  which  he  has  been  reared. 

The  research  is  a  study  of  certain  child-rearing  practices  and  their  effect 
upon  adult  personalities  and  behavior.  But  it  differs  from  most  other 
studies  having  the  same  objective.  These  other  investigations  generally 
assume  that  parents,  in  bringing  up  children,  have  theories  as  to  what  is 
good  and  bad  for  a  child's  development  and  that  these  theories  are  in- 
fluenced by  unconscious  motives  and  anxieties.  The  work  of  Dr.  Whiting 
and  his  group,  on  the  other  hand,  is  based  on  the  idea  that  differences  in 
child-rearing  practices  are  imposed  by  differences  in  certain  aspects  of  the 
physical  and  social  environment. 

A  good  deal  of  evidence  has  been  collected  in  support  of  the  Whiting 
group's  idea.  Much  of  it  comes  from  analyses  of  data  collected  by  other 
anthropologists  in  earlier  studies,  but  the  results  are  supported  by  more 
recent  field  investigations,  of  which  the  most  important  is  the  Six-Culture 
Study  directed  by  Dr.  Whiting,  Irvin  L.  Child  of  Yale,  and  William  W. 
Lambert  of  Cornell.  For  this  work,  six  pairs  of  anthropologists  spent  a 
year  observing  child-rearing  practices  in  six  widely  different  cultures:  a 
Gusii  community  in  Kenya,  Africa;  a  Mextcc  Indian  community  in  the 
state  of  Oaxaca,  Mexico;  a  community  of  Tarongans  in  the  Philippines;  a 
village  of  Okinawans;  a  neighborhood  of  families  of  the  Rajput  caste  in 

155 


the  province  of  Uttar  Pradesh,  India;  and  agioupof  New  England  families 
in  a  village  identified  as  "Orchard  Town  "  The  fieldwork,  done  in  the 
mid-1950's,  was  financed  by  the  Ford  Foundation;  the  analysis  of  the 
results,  partly  completed,  is  being  suppoited  by  Dr.  Whiting's  grant  from 
the  institute. 

John  Whiting  is  professor  of  social  anthropology  at  Harvard;  his  co- 
principal  investigator,  who  is  also  his  wife,  Dr.  Beatiice  B.  Whiting,  is  a 
lecturer  in  the  same  department.  They  plan  to  spend  a  year  in  Africa 
directing  field  studies  to  add  to  and  verify  the  most  important  of  the 
observations  and  inlerpietations  made  by  themselves  and  their  fellow 

workeis. 

"Our  aim,"  John  Whiting  explains,  "is  to  investigate  the  process  by 
which  a  child  learns  the  moral  rules  of  his  society  in  such  a  way  that  he 
will  not  deviate  from  them  even  when  the  piobabilities  of  his  getting 
caught  are  minimal-in  other  words,  the  inculcation  of  self-contiol." 

He  believes  a  high  degree  of  self-contiol  may  have  a  variety  of  causes, 
among  them  (a)  an  exaggerated  and  paranoid  fear  of  others,  (/;)  belief  in 
an  all-seeing  and  all-powerful  God  who  is  concerned  with  the  moral  be- 
havior of  mankind,  and  (c)  effective  training  for  the  parental  role.  The 
investigators  are  particularly  interested  in  this  third  cause  and  have  given  it 
the  bulk  of  their  attention.  By  "training"  they  do  not  mean  a  series  of 
instiuctions  and  examples  specifically  intended  to  help  fit  a  girl  to  cairy 
out  a  woman's  iole,  or  a  boy  a  man's;  they  mean,  most  of  all,  the  process 
by  which  a  child  identifies  with  an  adult  and,  therefore,  consciously  and 
unconsciously,  tries  to  be  like  him,  and  they  also  mean  the  characteristics 
of  a  society  that  affect  this  process. 

The  work  suggests  how  diffeiences  in  ceitain  child-rearing  customs  lead 
to  differences  in  certain  adult  characteristics.  It  throws  light  on  the 
process  of  learning,  and  in  doing  so  points  to  what  may  be  a  fundamental 
cause  of  much  juvenile  delinquency,  "Many  of  the  casualties  in  our  mental 
hospitals  and  jails,"  says  Whiting,  "piobably  would  have  been  spared  if  we 
had  had  a  better  understanding  of  the  complex  process  of  socialization," 
That's  what  he  is  trying  to  provide. 

The  investigators'  main  findings  and  conjectures  are  summarized  below 
and  presented  in  more  detail  in  later  sections  of  this  report. 


A  Summary  of  the  Results 

1 .  Cross-cultural  surveys  offer  a  support  for  John  Whiting's  basic  hypo- 
thesis-that  status  envy  is  a  prime  force  in  the  development  for  personal- 
ity. A  child  most  envies  the  person  who  can  withhold  the  resources  he 
values  most  highly,  the  hypothesis  says,  and  therefore,  he  tries  to  identify 
with  this  person.  In  monogamous  societies  where  the  father  is  home,  a 
male  child  generally  identifies  with  the  father.  But  where  the  father  is  not 
home,  and  particularly  in  societies  where  mother  and  child  sleep  together 
-and  the  father  elsewhere-during  a  long  post  partum  sex  taboo,  a  boy's 
primary  identification  may  well  be  feminine.  The  evidence  for  this  is 

156 


found  in  (a)  the  results  of  various  tests  for  cross-sex  identity;  (Z?)  a  display 
of  "hypermasculinity"— a  bending  ovei  backward  to  prove  themselves 
men— among  certain  gioups  of  boys  from  father-absent  01  father-weak 
homes;  (c)  the  practice  of  strenuous  male  puberty  rites-considered  a  type 
of  brainwashing-in  societies  whose  household  structure  and  other  factors 
would  lead  to  the  prediction  that  a  boy's  identification  would  be  with 
women.  The  investigatois  believe  that  these  findings  may  explain  much  of 
the  abeirant  behavior  in  our  own  society. 

2.  Most  monogamous  societies  rated  high  on  an  index  of  guilt;  most 
polygynous  societies,  low.  John  Whiting  believes  that  in  a  monogamous 
household,  a  boy  is  moie  likely  to  develop  a  conscience  based  on  his 
father's,  and   that  a  man's  conscience  is  stronger  and  more  unforgiving 
than  a  woman's. 

3.  Training  against  aggression  tends  to  be  most  severe  in  families  where 
the  household  comprises  not  only  parents  and  children  but  also  a  number 
of  theii  kinfolk-  The  investigators  believe  that  severe  Liaining  stems  from 
the  desire  of  the  adults  to  steer  clear  of  unpleasantness  likely  to  arise  from 
the  children's  fights. 

4.  Tiaining  for  independence,  too,  appears  to  be  related  to  household 
structure.  As  measured  by  early  weaning  and  by  reduced  contact  between 
mother  and  child,  it  is  found  most  often  in  nuclear  households— those 
comprised  of  the  father,  the  mother,  and   the  children.  Such  training, 
Whiting  thinks,  probably  (a}  leads  a  child  to  grow  up  with  the  urge  to  be 
successful  and  (b}  is  necessary  to  enable  a  couple  to  establish  their  home. 

5.  Evidence  is  offered  that  the  type  of  task  assigned  a  young  child- 
under  10-affects  his  peisonality.  The  most  responsible  children  in  the 
six-culture  study  were  those  who  had  to  take  care  of  younger  children, 
and  this  babysitting  job  seemed  to  be  related  to  the  amount  of  outside 
work  the  mother  had  to  do. 

6.  Eveiy where  in  the  six-culture  study,  boys  were  rougher  than  girls 
and  wandered  farther  from  home,  and  girls  rated  higher  on  a  responsibility 
measure.  Girls  were  more  responsible  than  boys  (and  women  in  general 
show  more  sensitivity  than  men),  Beatrice  Whiting  surmises,  because  they 
were  more  often  assigned  the  care  of  younger  children. 

7.  A  probable  link  is  reported,  too,  between  the  kind  of  tasks  assigned 
a   child  and    the   kind   of  supernatural   power   the  culture  believes  in. 
Societies  where  a  child's  work  is  to  care  for  children  or  cattle,  with  the 
results  of  failure  being  immediate  and  dramatic  and  punishment  severe, 
tend  also  to  be  societies  where  the  supurnaturals  punish  a  person  here  and 
now.  Where  the  main  tasks  arc  household  chores  and  schoolwoik,  there  is 
a  tendency  to  believe  in  supernatural  who  punish  only  in  an  afterlife, 

8.  Some   evidence   is  found   that  polygyny  arises  from  a  long  post 
partum  sex  taboo,  and  that  the  taboo  in  turn  is  related  to  a  diet  so  poor 
that  the  mother  fears  to  conceive  until  the  nursing  child  has  been  weaned, 
which  may  not  be  for  several  years, 

9.  Certain    kinds    of    stress    during    in  fancy -including    inoculation, 
circumcision,  and  periodic  separation  from  the  mother  during  the  first 
days  of  life-appear  to  be  related  to  adult  stature.  In  societies  practicing 

157 


these  kinds  of  stress,  the  average  adult  is  significantly  taller  than  in  other 
societies.  The  investigators  hope  to  learn  whether  or  not  psychological 
differences  also  occur— as  they  do  in  rats. 

The  investigators  emphasize  that  the  findings  summarized  here  are 
based  almost  entirely  on  studies  of  cultures,  many  of  them  primitive, 
othei  than  our  own,  but  theie  is  evidence  that  some  of  the  conclusions— 
especially  concerning  the  effect  of  the  fathers  absence-apply  here,  too. 
Study  of  other  cultures  is  an  essential  means  of  discovering  universal 
tiuths  about  personality  and  behavior. 


Status  Envy  as  the  Unconscious  Motivation  for  Behavior 

The  most  irnpoitant  mechanism  shaping  the  development  of  a  child's 
conscience-or,  in  psychoanalytic  terms,  superego-and  an  important 
factor  in  his  behavior  throughout  life,  John  Whiting  believes,  is  status 
envy.  According  to  this  hypothesis,  the  child  envies  the  person  who  with- 
holds resources  from  him  01  deprives  him  of  them.  The  child  thereupon 
seeks  to  identify  himself  with  this  person;  that  is,  tries  to  learn  to  act  like 
this  person  so  that  he,  too,  can  become  a  controller  of  resources. 

Resources  include  food,  water,  love,  power,  freedom  from  pain- 
anything  somebody  wants.  Every  society  has  a  status  system  under  which 
peisons  at  a  certain  level  have  privileged  access  to  resomces  while  persons 
at  another  level  do  not.  The  conditions  for  status  envy  arise  even  during 
infancy,  for  no  matter  how  everloving  a  parent  may  wish  to  be,  there  arc 
times  when  he  or  she  must  withhold  something  the  baby  wants. 

As  Whiting  views  it,  the  oedipal  situation,  so  prominent  in  psycho- 
analytic theory,  is  merely  a  special  case  of  the  status  envy  hypothesis. 
Under  the  psychoanalytic  interpretation,  the  boy  child  vies  with  his  father 
for  the  mother's  love;  under  Whiting's  interpretation,  the  boy  child  simply 
envies  his  mother— and  therefore,  identifies  with  her  for  a  time—because 
she  can  dispense  to  other  persons  a  resource,  love,  he  wants  for  himself. 

The  more  a  child  envies  the  status  of  another  with  respect  to  the 
control  of  a  given  resource,  Whiting  believes,  the  more  he  will  practice 
that  role-openly  perhaps,  but  certainly  covertly,  meaning  that  he  will 
indulge  in  a  fantasy  in  which  he  sees  himself  as  the  envied  person  control- 
ling and  consuming  the  valued  resources  of  which  he  has  been  deprived.  It 
is  this  fantasy  of  being  someone  other  than  himself  that  Whiting  defines  as 
identification.  As  a  simple  example,  when  a  child  wants  to  stay  up  late, 
but  his  parents  make  him  go  to  bed,  he  may  say  to  himself,  "I  wish  I  were 
grown  up.  Perhaps  if  I  acted  as  they  do,  I  would  be."  And  he  thinks  about 
grownup  behavior  as  he  goes  to  sleep. 

Further,  the  child  will  tend  to  manipulate  resources  as  his  parents  have 
manipulated  them  with  him;  for  instance,  if  he  has  been  given  resources 
when  he  had  a  special  need  for  thern-such  as  solace  when  he  was  hurt  and 
assurance  when  lie  was  frightened-he  will  respond  the  same  way  to  his 
fellows  when  they  are  hurt  or  frightened.  The  child  also  will  tend  to 
respond  to  the  naughty  behavior  of  others  as  his  parents  have  responded 

158 


to  his;  and,  having  taken  on  the  parents'  role  through  status  envy,  he  will 
punish  himself  as  well  as  others. 

In  some  cultures  the  investigatois  suspect  a  strong  tendency  for  a  child 
to  identify  with  his  mother  during  at  least  the  early  years.  Depending  on 
who  controls  which  resources,  however,  he  may  well  identify  with  both 
parents.  If,  for  example,  the  mother  has  primary  control  of  food  and  love 
whereas  the  father  controls  the  power  to  administer  or  to  withhold  physi- 
cal punishment,  the  theory  predicts  that  the  child  will  identify  with  both 
parents  but  with  respect  to  different  lesources.  He  will  take  after  his 
mother  with  respect  to  love  and  affection;  after  his  father  with  respect  to 
power  and  authority. 

All  cultures  have  rules  concerning  status  that  differ  considerably  from 
the  desires  of  a  growing  child.  For  instance,  a  child  is  supposed  to  act 
neither  like  a  baby  nor  a  grownup;  a  boy  is  not  supposed  to  be  a  sissy,  nor 
a  girl  a  tomboy.  Hence  the  desire  to  play  certain  roles  of  envied  statuses 
may  lie  latent  for  years.  "It  has  often  been  noted,"  Whiting  observes, 
"that  a  mother  will  frequently  respond  to  her  fiist  child  exactly  as  her 
own  mother  had  treated  her,  even  though  she  is  not  aware  of  practicing 
such  behavior,  and,  in  fact,  may  even  have  vociferously  sworn  that  she  was 
going  to  bring  up  her  children  differently.  This  suggests  that  covert 
practice  of  envied  roles  may  often  be  disguised  and  unconscious." 

The  idea  that  children  identify  with  the  persons  close  to  them  who 
control  valued  resources  is  not  new,  but  John  Whiting  has  done  more  than 
anyone  else  to  seek  confirmatory  evidence  and  has  supplied  the  name  by 
which  the  hypothesis  is  now  generally  known. 


The  Problem  of  a  Conflict  Over  Sex  Identity 

Development  of  the  status-envy  hypothesis  has  pushed  Whiting  into 
fascinating  speculations  for  which  he  has  tried  to  find  a  solid  basis  in  the 
child-training  arrangements  and  the  behavior  characteristics  of  numerous 
cultures,  including  some  in  the  United  States. 

The  speculations  so  far  have  centered  on  cross-sex  identity.  If  a  mother 
has  had  control  of  all  the  resources  a  young  boy  values,  Whiting's  theory 
says  he  will  envy  her  status  and  try  to  perform  her  role.  Part  of  the 
behavior  that  he  seeks  to  emulate,  though,  is  sex-typed  and  forbidden. 
"Such  a  boy  finds  himself  in  dire  conflict,"  the  investigator  says.  "Ho  may 
practice  feminine-role  behavior  despite  the  sanctions  against  it,  or,  if  the 
sanctions  are  too  compelling,  inhibit  his  impulses  to  perform  such  be- 
havior. The  theory  predicts,  however,  that  he  would  continue  to  piactice 
covertly  and  would  thus  have  a  feminine  self-image," 

As  a  test  of  these  views,  the  investigators  analyzed  sleeping  arrangement 
in  a  sample  of  64  societies.  If  a  boy  sleeps  alone  with  his  mother  during 
his  first  years,  covering  the  nursing  period,  the  reasoning  went,  he  will 
come  to  consider  her  as  all-important  and,  because  she  sometimes  with- 
holds resources,  as  the  person  to  be  envied.  So  his  primary  identification, 
or  the  type  acquired  during  infancy,  will  be  feminine.  If  he  then,  after 

159 


weaning,  enters  a  world  in  which  men  are  obviously  the  important  per- 
sons, able  to  bestow  01  to  withhold  the  most  valued  lesources,  the  boy's 
secondary  identification,  acquired  during  childhood,  will  be  masculine. 

In  such  a  case,  the  reasoning  continued,  the  boy's  conflict  over  sex 
identity  should  be  especially  severe,  so  severe,  in  fact,  that  society  would 
step  in  to  resolve  it  and  assure  him  beyond  any  doubt  of  his  masculinity. 
This  would  be  done  through  elaborate  male  initiation  rites  at  puberty, 
including  circumcision  and  tests  of  strength  and  endurance— all  serving,  in 
the  investigators'  words,  "to  brainwash  the  primary  feminine  identity  and 
to  establish  firmly  the  secondary  male  identity." 

Native  theoiy  is  offered  in  support  of  this  interpretation.  Most  societies 
having  male  initiation  rites,  the  investigators  report,  have  one  word  refer- 
ring to  all  women  and  uninitiated  boys  and  another  word  referring  only  to 
initiated  males.  In  these  societies  a  boy  is  born  twice;  first  into  woman- 
child  status  and  then,  at  puberty,  into  the  status  of  manhood. 

Fiom  the  sample  studied,  13  societies  were  found  to  have  such  rites,  In 
all  13,  mother  and  infant  slept  together  and  alone.  (For  the  most  pait, 
these  were  polygynous  societies,  with  a  long  post  partum  sex  taboo.  When 
a  wife  had  a  nursing  baby,  the  father  slept  in  another  wife's  house  or  in 
the  men's  quarters.)  And  12  of  the  13  were  patrilocal  societies,  in  which 
the  domestic  unit  comprises  a  group  of  closely  related  male  and  a  group  of 
unrelated  females  brought  in  from  other  villages  as  wives;  in  such  societies 
prestige  and  power  are  clearly  vested  in  the  men. 

There  are  numerous  cases,  however,  where  the  secondary  as  well  as  the 
primary  identification  should  be  feminine  because  the  child  not  only 
sleeps  alone  with  his  mother  during  infancy  but  also  grows  up  in  a  matri- 
local  world,  controlled  by  his  mother,  his  aunts,  and  his  maternal  grand- 
mother. Such  a  society,  the  investigators  reasoned,  should  give  a  man 
some  means  to  act  out,  symbolically  at  least,  the  female  lolc,  and  they 
found  such  a  means  in  the  custom  known  as  the  couvade.  Under  this 
custom,  during  the  period  just  preceding  or  following  the  biith  of  a  child, 
the  husband  takes  to  bed,  fasts  or  limits  himself  to  ccitain  foods,  under- 
goes a  puiificalion  ceremony,  and  in  general  accepts  the  same  attention  as 
his  wife. 

Childbirth  epitomizes  the  uniquely  feminine  part  of  a  woman's  role. 
"When  a  man  attempts  to  participate  In  the  birth  of  his  child  by  closely 
imitating  the  behavior  of  his  wife,1'  say  the  researchers,  "this  should  be  a 
good  index  of  his  wish  to  act  out  the  feminine  role  and  thus  symbolically 
to  be  in  part  a  woman." 

The  Harvard  anthropologists  found  the  couvade  a  common  piactice  in 
12  societies  in  their  sample.  In  10  of  these,  mother  and  infant  slept 
together,  and  in  nine,  because  of  the  matrilocal  residence  pattern,  women 
even  after  the  nursing  period  controlled  the  resources  most  valued  by  a 
growing  child, 

"Masculine  Protest"  Behavior  and  the  Absent  Father 

In  another  test  of  the  theory,  a  husband-and-wifc  team  from  Harvard, 
Robert  L,  and  Ruth  H.  Munroe,  recently  studied  the  Black  Carib  of 

160 


British  Honduras.  This  group  springs  from  escaped  Negro  slaves  who 
assimilated  the  culture  of  the  natives,  the  Island  Carib,  of  St.  Vincent,  in 
the  Caribbean,  and  then  spread  over  the  coastline  of  the  Gulf  of  Hon- 
duras, in  Central  America. 

As  practiced  by  the  Black  Carib,  the  couvade  involves  a.  number  of 
restrictions  on  the  father's  oidinary  behavior,  among  them  taboos  on 
fishing  and  on  extramarital  intercourse.  If  the  restrictions  are  violated,  the 
Carib  believes  that  the  new  bom  infant  will  get  sick.  But  some  fathers,  the 
intensive-couvade  group,  observe  many  taboos;  others,  the  weak-couvade 
group,  only  a  few.  The  approximately  50  fathers  studied  fell  about  evenly 
into  the  two  groups. 

The  investigators  hypothesized  that  the  gioups  had  been  brought  up 
differently;  specifically,  that  the  intensive-couvade  males  had  been  under 
stronger  female  influence.  This  turned  out  to  be  correct;  during  the  first  3 
years  of  life  the  intensive-couvade  group  had  spent  more  than  twice  as 
much  time  as  the  others  in  a  household  whose  only  adults  were  women. 

But  were  these  men  really  more  feminine,  psychologically,  than  the 
otheis?  Cross-sex  identity  is  a  tricky  thing  to  measure,  but  Whiting  and 
some  fellow  workers  had  attempted  to  do  it  earlier— and  successfully,  they 
think-with  the  so-called  magic  man  test.  In  this  test,  the  subject  is  told  to 
suppose  that  he  can  be  anybody  he  wishes  and  is  then  asked  who  he 
would  most  like  to  be-a  father,  a  mother,  a  son,  a  daughter,  and  so  on. 
Then  he  is  asked  to  make  further  choices.  American  males  brought  up  in 
the  usual  household,  with  both  father  and  mother  attending  to  them  from 
infancy,  tend  to  choose  all  the  male  statuses  fiist;  American  females,  all 
the  female  statuses.  But  the  earliei  study  had  found  that  among  young- 
sters with  good  reason  to  envy  the  status  of  the  opposite  sex,  a  number 
did  make  some  cross-sex  choices.  Among  the  Black  Carib,  the  Munroes 
found,  males  who  practiced  an  intensive  couvade  chose  to  be  a  mother,  a 
daughter,  or  a  baby  girl  significantly  more  often  than  males  who  observed 
the  couvade  only  weakly. 

The  Munroes  also  used  the  Semantic  Differential  Test,  in  which  the 
subject  is  given  a  number  of  scales  having  contrasting  adjectives  at  each 
end,  like  good  and  bad,  fast  and  slow,  and  strong  and  weak,  and  is  asked 
to  use  the  scales  to  describe  various  persons.  The  men  who  observed  the 
couvade  most  strongly  tended  to  rate  themselves  on  these  scales  the  same 
way  they  rated  women. 

There  were  still  other  discriminating  factors.  Black  Carib  men  and 
women  show  certain  linguistic  differences;  men,  for  example,  tend  to  use 
a  different  word  than  women  for  "yesterday."  Asked  to  translate  from 
English  to  Carib,  the  intensive  couvade  men  used  female  words  signifi- 
cantly more  often  than  the  other  men.  Further,  when  their  wives  were 
pregnant,  the  intensive  couvade  men  experienced  many  more  pregnancy 
symptoms  than  the  others.  Male  pregnancy  symptoms,  which  have  been 
found  in  all  societies  where  they  have  been  asked  about,  are  believed  by 
these  investigators  to  have  the  same  psychological  implications  as  the 
couvade  itself. 

161 


If  all  this  seems  a  little  beside  the  point,  it  may  be  recalled  that  numer- 
ous investigatois  have  found  links  between  family  conditions  and  juvenile 
delinquency.  The  Harvard  gioup  is  now  offering  evidence  that  the  specific 
problem  is  the  mother-present,  father-absent  or  father-weak  household 
structure  during  the  first  years  of  life,  because  in  such  a  home  a  boy 
originally  identifies  with  women  and  then  later  on  may  go  overboard  to 
prove  his  masculinity.  This  cross-cultural  reseaich  may,  then,  explain  an 
underlying  cause  of  much  juvenile  delinquency,  alcoholism,  and  other 
aberrant  behavior. 

Beatrice  Whiting  puts  the  matter  this  way; 

If  during  the  first  2  or  3  years  of  life  a  child  is  constantly  with  his  mother  and 
infrequently  sees,  and  is  handled  by,  his  father,  he  will  identify  strongly  with  his 
mother  and  not  with  his  father;  in  short,  if  he  is  a  boy  he  will  have  a  cross-sex 
identification.  If,  later,  in  life,  he  is  involved  in  a  world  in  which  men  are  perceived  to 
be  more  prestigeful  and  powerful  than  women,  he  will  be  thrown  into  conflict.  He  will 
develop  a  strong  need  to  reject  his  underlying  female  identity.  This  may  lead  to  an 
overdeter mined  attempt  to  prove  his  masculinity,  manifested  by  a  preoccupation  with 
physical  strength  and  athletic  prowess,  or  attempts  to  demonstrate  daring  and  valor,  or 
behavior  that  is  violent  and  aggressive, 

A  boy's  attempt  to  prove  his  masculinity  has  been  used  to  explain  the 
high  rate  of  juvenile  delinquency  among  Negroes  in  lower-class  neighbor- 
hoods. John  Whiting  points  in  particularly  to  a  study  concluding  that  the 
Negro  juvenile  gang  member  rejects  femininity  in  every  form-"and  he 
sees  it  in  women  and  in  effeminate  men,  in  laws  and  morals  and  religion,  in 
schools  and  occupational  striving."1  And  a  study  of  the  correlates  of 
crime  in  48  societies  found  that  lack  or  limitation  of  opportunity  for 
young  boys  to  form  an  identification  with  their  fathers  was  associated 
with  a  higher  frequency  of  theft,  assault,  rape,  murder,  and  other  crimes.2 

The  new  developments  by  the  Harvard  group  are  the  elaboration  of  the 
status-envy  hypothesis  as  an  explanation  of  masculine-protest  behavior 
and  the  finding  of  such  behavior,  linked  with  father-missing  households, 
dining  field  studies,  As  one  important  example,  the  Munroes  found  it 
among  the  Black  Carib.  The  men  who  practiced  the  couvade  most 
intensively  were  described  by  the  people  of  the  town  as  braver  men, 
heavier  drinkers,  and  more  frequent  cursors  than  the  others.  "In  the  day- 
to-day  situation,"  the  investigators  report,  "the  intcnsive-couvade  men 
were  prototypes  of  the  rugged  male." 

Carrying  their  work  to  the  United  States,  the  Munroes  then  examined 
the  exhaustive  records  of  200  men  whose  wives  had  given  birth  at  a 
Boston  hospital.  The  records  were  exhaustive  because  they  were  part  of 
the  comprehensive,  long-time  perinatal  study -an  inquiry  into  early  condi- 
tions associated  with  later  defects-sponsored  by  the  National  Institute  of 
Neurological  Diseases  and  Blindness.  The  men  were  white  and  from  a 
variety  of  racial  stocks  and  socio economic  levels.  Forty  percent  of  the 


1  Rohrer,  J,  H.,  and  Edmonson,  M.  S.  "The  Eighth  Generation."  New  York:  Harper 
1960. 

"'Bacon,  Margaret  K.,  Child,  I.  K.,  and  Barry,  II.,  HI.  A  cioss-cultural  study  of 
correlates  of  crime./.  Abnorm,  Soc.  Psycho!,,  1963,66. 

162 


\ 


husbands  reported  that  they  had  experienced  common  pregnancy 
symptoms— foi  example,  nausea,  vomiting,  toothache,  and  food  cravings. 
About  30  of  these  men  and  their  wives  were  studied,  as  were  a  group 
similar  in  size  and  general  characteristics  except  that  the  husbands  had  had 
no  pregnancy  symptoms. 

The  men  who  had  had  symptoms,  the  team  reports,  tended  to  respond 
more  like  females,  as  compared  with  the  other  men,  on  a  number  of 
measures— on  the  way  they  completed  a  drawing,  foi  example,  on  their 
preferences  in  television  piograms,  and  on  their  attitudes  toward  children. 
The  men  with  symptoms  had  been  happier  about  the  coining  event,  and 
they  usually  spent  moie  time  feeding,  bathing,  and  caring  for  their 
children  than  the  other  men.  But  on  a  scale  dealing  with  typically  female 
activities,  such  as  cooking,  washing  dishes,  and  setting  the  table,  the 
symptomatology  group  reported  significantly  less  participation  than  the 
otheis.  This  indicated,  the  investigators  believe,  a  shift  towaul  hypei- 
masculinity  in  the  case  of  activities  clearly  associated  with  the  other  sex. 

But  the  payoff  is  this.  The  men  were  asked  if,  during  their  early  years, 
their  fathers  had  been  gone  from  home,  permanently  or  foi  long  periods 
of  time.  In  many  cases  they  had  been,  because  of  divorce,  death,  or  jobs 
that  had  taken  them  out  of  town.  The  men  with  pregnancy  symptoms  had 
experienced  a  significantly  greater  loss  in  this  respect  than  the  others. 

In  sum,  male  pregnancy  symptoms  have  been  linked  by  the  Munroes  to 
cross-sex  identity  in  two  widely  different  cultures.  This  suggests  to  the 
investigators  and  their  Harvard  associates  that  these  symptoms  may 
eventually  prove  to  be  a  widely  usable  measure  of  a  man's  identification 
with  the  opposite  sex.  If  so,  the  Harvard  group's  attempt  to  prove  or 
disprove  the  idea  that  a  peison's  primary  identification  is  with  the  adult 
who  controls  the  resources  he  most  wants  in  early  childhood  would  be 
considerably  cased.  This  is  because  the  results  of  tests  to  uncover  a 
person's  primary  identification  are  hard  to  verify.  The  presence  or  absence 
of  pregnancy  symptoms  in  adult  males  would  be  a  welcome  check  on  the 
other  tests  and  hopefully  could  even  serve  alone, 

Several  investigators  elscwhcie  have  compared  the  families  of 
Norwegian  sailors,  gone  from  home,  often  for  2  years  or  more,  with 
families  in  which  the  fathcis  were  present.  The  mothers  in  the  sailor 
families  were  found  to  lay  greater  stress  on  obcdicnce-instead  of 
happiness  and  self-realixation—than  the  other  mothers.  And  the  boys 
tended  to  be  infantile  and  dependent  and  to  behave  in  overly  masculine 
ways.  John  Whiting  hopes  to  learn  someday  what  happens  to  these  boys. 
He  would  not  be  surprised  if,  during  adolescence,  they  become  sailors 
themselves.  Sailing  is  an  occupation  suitable  for  a  man  who  places  a  high 
value  on  obedience,  he  says;  further,  on  an  extended  voyage,  it  permits  a 
man  to  engage  in  certain  work-such  as  cleaning  one's  quarters  and 
sewing— associated  with  the  female  role.  And  Whiting  suspects  that  during 
their  first  voyage  these  boys  may  have  to  undergo  a  rather  severe  initiation 
ceremony— analogous  to  the  male  puberty  rites  in  many  primitive 
societies,  particularly  those  where  a  young  child  has  unusual  opportunity 
to  identify  with  females. 

163 


The  idea  that  the  pubeity  ntes  serve  to  stiengthen  a  boy's  masculine 
identification  wilt  he  checked  in  the  field  during  the  Whitings'  African 
yeai.  The  rites-held  in  what  anthropologists  call  bush  schools,  remote 
fiom  the  settlement -continue  for  days  and  even  weeks  and  can  be 
extremely  exhausting  physically  and  psychologically.  In  one  reported 
case,  the  young  men  returning  to  their  village  were  at  fust  unable  to 
lecogm/e  its  inhabitants.  Whiting  pioposes  to  administer  a  number  of 
psychological  tests  to  candidates  shortly  befoie  the  initiation  ceiemonies, 
and  then  again  afterward.  He  expects  to  find  a  numbei  of  cross-sex 
identifications  the  fiist  time;veiy  few,  if  any,  the  second. 

In  addition  to  household  stuicture  during  infancy,  this  reseaich  group 
emphasizes,  prominent  elements  affecting  adult  personality  aie  the 
relative  importance  of  the  sexes  as  a  child  grows  up  and  the  personal 
attributes  a  culture  values  most  highly  Among  people  in  the  lower 
socioeconomic  levels  in  the  United  States,  the  Whitings  note,  thegiowing 
boy  is  likely  to  peiceive  men  as  clearly  the  more  powerful  and  the  more  to 
be  envied;  so  if  his  primary  identification  is  feminine,  his  conflict  should 
be  unusually  severe.  But  among  the  middle  and  upper  classes,  male 
dominance  is  less  clear,  so  the  conflict  for  a  boy  who  has  made  an  initial 
feminine  identification  may  be  small.  Beatrice  Whiting  refers  to  a  study  of 
college  boys  who  spent  the  first  2  years  of  their  lives  with  their  mothers, 
their  fathers  being  overseas  in  World  War  II.3  These  boys  were  found  to 
have  feminine  attributes  but  to  be  neither  anxious  nor  defensive  about 
them.  The  suggested  reason  is  that  academic  and  intellectual  circles  place  a 
high  value  on  sensitivity,  aesthetic  interests,  verbal  ability,  and  certain 
othei  chanicteiistics  usually  considered  mote  typical  of  the  female  than 
the  male. 

Answers  must  still  be  sought  to  the  following  questions,  as  listed  by 
John  Whiting  and  a  former  associate,  Roger  V.  Burton,  now  at  NIMH: 

Are  theie  times  when  the  absence  of  a  father  is  more  critical  than  at 
other  times? 

How  long  docs  it  take  for  a  child  to  establish  identity? 

How  do  the  effects  of  a  weak  father  compare  with  those  of  an  absent 
father? 

What  is  the  effect  of  an  absent  father  on  the  development  of  a  girl? 

Violence  and  Aggression  in  the  Six  Cultures 

Further  support  for  the  status-envy  idea  comes  fiom  Beatrice  Whiting's 
recent  analysis  of  violence  among  the  societies  covered  in  the  six-culture 
study.  Two  of  these  societics-the  Gusii  tribe  of  Kenya,  Africa,  and 
members  of  the  Rajput  caste  in  Uttar  Pradesh,  India-were  found  to  have, 
as  compated  with  the  other  four,  an  unusual  amount  of  violence.  The  men 
in  the  Rajput  sample  of  a  few  dozen  families  had  been  involved  in  59 


3CaTlsmith,  Karolyn  Kucfcenberg.  Effect  of  early  father  absence  on  scholastic  apti- 
tude. Ph.D.  dissertation,  1963,  Harvard. 

164 


court  cases,  1 7  of  them  concerning  incidents  that  had  taken  u  violent  turn, 
leading  in  thice  instances  to  homicide.  The  Gusii  sample -most  of  whose 
men  had  been  involved  in  at  least  one  court  case— remembered  seven 
instances  of  violence  between  men  they  could  name  and  numcious  others 
involving  men  they  could  not  name.  The  other  societies,  in  conliast, 
reported  very  few  if  any  such  instances.  Among  the  Okinawans,  the 
study's  fieldworkcis  commented  upon  "the  absence  of  ciime  and  the  low 
incidence  of  quarrels,  disputes,  and  brawls."  The  Mixtecans  seemed  to  fear 
aggression;  children  were  told  that  if  they  became  angry  and  then  ate, 
they  would  die.  In  Luzon,  the  Philippines,  there  was  a  good  deal  of 
quaireling  but  scarcely  any  physical  violence.  In  "Orchard  Town,"  New 
England,  as  in  these  other  cultures,  assault  and  homicide  cases  involving 
the  people  studied  01  anyone  they  knew  by  name  were  exceedingly  rare. 

Besides  the  impression  that  the  Rajputs  and  the  Gusii  were  unusually 
aggressive,  something  else  differentiated  them  from  the  people  in  the 
other  four  cultures.  In  neither  of  the  aggressive  gioups  was  it  customaty 
for  husband  and  wife  to  work,  play,  eat,  01  even-in  the  sense  of  oc- 
cupying the  same  room-sleep  together. 

Most  of  the  Gusii  in  the  sample  studied  are  polygynous.  The  huts  they 
build  foi  each  of  their  wives  have  a  special  section,  with  its  own  entrance, 
for  the  exclusive  use  of  the  husband.  The  husband  either  rotates  among 
these  huts  or  sleeps  in  a  special  house,  which  he  may  share  with  his 
unmarried  sons  who  have  undergone  the  puberty  riles.  He  seldom,  if  evei, 
visits  a  wife  with  a  nursing  baby. 

Among  the  Rajputs,  the  men  of  a  household  sleep  in  a  separate  struc- 
ture. When  a  husband  visits  his  wife,  they  go  to  an  unused  room,  or,  more 
likely,  a  deserted  part  of  the  courtyard,  within  whose  walls  the  wife 
spends  most  of  her  young  married  life,  along  with  her  female  in-laws. 
Aftei  a  child  is  born,  the  husband  is  not  supposed  to  visit  his  wife  for  2  or 
3  years,  a  taboo  which  is  by  no  means  umvcisally  observed. 

In  both  of  these  cultures,  then,  the  world  of  Ihe  infant  is  largely  a 
women's  world.  For  his  first  several  years,  the  young  child  sees  his  father 
only  infrequently.  But  when  he  is  able  to  move  beyond  the  immediate 
circle  of  his  mother,  the  child  begins  to  perceive  that  the  really  important 
people  are  the  men.  "The  Rajput  3-y car-old  must  notice  that  the  women 
get  down  on  the  floor  and  cover  their  heads  every  time  a  male  enters  the 
courtyard,"  notes  Beatrice  Whiting.  "The  hungry  3-year-old  in  Nyansongo 
(the  Gusii  community)  must  have  looked  with  longing  «t  the  basket  of 
food  prepared  for  his  father.  He  must  have  learned  that  his  father  has  a 
private  world  in  his  special  room  or  house.  He  knows  that  when  he  is  older 
he  will  sleep  with  his  brothers  and  his  father,  and  will  be  initiated  with 
other  boys  in  a  ceremony  from  which  all  women  are  excluded,"  Hence  it 
is  in  the  Rajput  and  Gusii  cultures  that  the  status-envy  theory  would 
predict  protest  masculinity,  and  it  is  in  these  two  cultures  lliat  it  seems  to 
be  most  evident,  as  indicated  by  the  extent  of  physical  violence,  "For  the 
little  boys  brought  up  in  the  other  four  societies/'  the  investigator 
observes,  "the  problem  of  male  control  in  the  world  of  the  3-  to 
6-year-olds  in  not  theoretically  relevant,  as  they  have  already  had  a  chance 


to  identify  with  males  They  have  seen  men  and  women  mteiact  in 
intimate  settings  since  biith.  Moieover,  even  if  they  have  made  a  sttong 
feminine  identification,  there  will  be  less  conflict  later  in  life  since  the 
importance  of  men  and  women  will  be  more  nearly  equal  and  the  contrast 
between  the  behavior  and  personal  profiles  of  the  sexes  will  be  less.1' 


The  Mormons,  the  Texans,  and  the  Zuni 

Status  envy  and  cross-sex  identity  presumably  apply  to  girls  as  well  as 
boys  but  have  been  little  tested  with  them.  In  one  of  the  very  few  studies 
including  tests  of  botli  sexes,  John  Whiting  and  a  group  of  associates 
observed  members  of  three  different  cultuies  living  close  to  one  another  in 
western  New  Mexico-Mormons,  homesteaders  from  Texas,  and  Zuni 
Indians.  The  field  work,  part  of  a  larger  investigation  supported  by  the 
Rockefcllei  Foundation,  was  clone  in  1950  but  the  analysis  was  completed 
only  recently. 

The  children  given  the  magic-man  test  weie  in  grades  thiec  to  six. 
Among  the  Mormon  gills,  23  percent  chose  to  be  males-reflecting,  the 
investigators  report,  the  lelatively  high  status  of  men  among  Mormons. 
Among  the  Zuni  boys,  10  peicent  chose  to  be  girls,  reflecting  the  relative- 
ly high  status  of  women  among  the  Zuni.  There  were  no  eross-scx  choices 
by  the  Texas  children,  which  came  as  no  surprise  because  Texan  men  and 
women  were  regarded  by  the  investigators  as  having  relatively  equal  status. 

The  effect  of  cross-sex  identification  among  some  of  the  Monnon 
girls-assuming  of  course  that  the  magic-man  test  is  a  valid  indicator  of 
it— is  not  known.  Bui  there  is  some  evidence  from  studies  of  lower-class 
families  by  other  investigatois  that  girls  brought  up  in  mother-child  house- 
holds become  more  dominant  and  aggressive  than  gills  reared  in  house- 
holds where  both  the  father  and  the  mother  are  present.  Perhaps  this  is 
the  result  of  a  secondary  identification,  Beatrice  Whiting  suggests,  arising 
from  the  perception  that  men  in  lower-class  neighborhoods  clearly  have 
more  power  than  women,  Or  perhaps  it  is  the  result  of  a  primaty  identifi- 
cation with  a  mother  who  has  some  masculine  characteristics  because, 
without  a  man,  she  has  to  fend  foi  herself. 

In  any  event,  the  New  Mexico  study  offers  interesting  examples  of  the 
relationship  between  the  structure  of  a  family  and  the  way  children  are 
brought  up. 

The  Zuni,  as  one  main  instance,  emphasized  rigid  training  for  the 
control  of  aggression.  Was  this  because  of  some  innate  love  of  harmony? 
Not  at  all,  the  investigators  say,  after  a  look  into  history.  The  Zuni 
originally  lived  in  single-family  houses  but  by  1300  these  had  been 
replaced  by  the  great  pueblos,  perhaps  because  of  invading  Apaches. 
Crowded  living  conditions  and,  especially,  the  requirement  that  several 
women  share  in  the  running  of  the  household,  led  to  an  emphasis  on 
harmony.  In  a  worldwide  sample  of  30  societies,  92  percent  of  those  with 
extended  families-this  is,  households  comprising  not  only  parents  and 
children  but  also  a  variety  of  relatives-rated  above  the  median  in  the 

166 


severity  with  which  aggressive  manisfestations  by  the  children  were 
punished.  Only  22  percent  of  the  nucleai  family  households— parents  and 
childien— were  equally  severe.  Punishment  for  aggression,  John  Whiting 
believes,  is  directly  tied  in  with  the  deshe  of  adult  kinfolk,  by  blood  or 
marriage,  to  steer  fiee  of  squabbles  likely  to  be  engendered  by  their 
children's  fights. 

This  view  gains  strength  from  a  recent  analysis  (by  Leigh  Minturn, 
"University  of  Illinois,  and  William  Lambert,  Cornell)  of  the  data 
concerning  the  mothers  in  the  six-culture  study.  All  the  mothers  show 
some  concern  for  the  quarrels  of  their  children,  but  the  Mixtec  mothers 
are  the  most  concerned,  and  the  New  England  mothers  the  least. 

The  adults  in  the  Mixtec  sample,  the  investigators  observe,  are  highly 
interdependent,  with  many  close  kinship  ties.  Brothers  and  their  families 
generally  live  around  a  common  courtyard,  where  the  children  play; 
relatives  look  to  one  another  for  help  with  their  work  and  for  financial 
assistance.  Consequently,  the  Mixtec  mothers  teach  their  children  to  be 
unaggressive.  Physical  punishment  is  used  to  punish  aggression  moie 
commonly  than  it  is  used  for  any  other  type  of  behavior.  Sometimes,  to 
prevent  fights,  children  are  even  kept  home  from  school.  One  result  is  that 
Mixtec  adults  are  unaggiessive  and  unusually  slow  to  take  offense. 

The  New  England  families,  on  the  other  hand,  are  the  only  group  whose 
membeis  are  not  living  next  door  to  relatives  and  whose  livelihood  does 
not  depend  upon  the  support  of  their  neighbors.  This  means  that  they  can 
ignore  their  neighbors  if  they  cannot  get  along  with  them;  it  also  means 
they  have  no  claims  of  kinship  to  cement  relationships  in  case  they  do 
want  to  be  friends  with  their  neighbors.  Under  these  circumstances,  a 
mother  is  likely  to  tell  her  child  to  go  play  with  somebody  else  if  he  can't 
get  along  with  the  children  next  door;  she  also  seems  reluctant  to 
complain  to  her  neighbois  on  behalf  of  hei  children. 

The  investigators  emphasize  that  New  England  mothcis  punish  children 
if  they  instigate  aggression  or  if  they  attack  younger  childien  even  though 
provoked.  More  than  any  other  group  of  mothers  in  the  study,  they  arc 
concerned  that  children  learn  the  rules  of  "fail  fighting"  and  the  occasions 
that  justify  retaliation. 

As  another  example  of  the  influence  of  household  structure  upon  child 
training  practices,  the  homesteaders  from  Texas,  in  the  earlier  study, 
exerted  early  and  strong  pressure  for  self-reliance  and  independence- 
pressure,  say  the  investigators,  that  does  not  spring  from  an  innate  desire 
for  their  children  to  be  successful  but  is  related,  like  the  Zimi  and  Mixtec 
training  against  aggression,  to  living  conditions.  In  the  Tcxans'  case,  events 
had  brought  a  swing  away  from  the  extended  family,  a  feature  of  Gieat 
Britain  in  Elizabethan  times.  The  male  head  of  this  extended  family  had 
been  in  full  control,  and  child-rearing  piactices  had  included  relatively  late 
weaning,  at  two  yeais,  and  swaddling,  which  hampered  movement. 
"Dependence  was  more  valued  than  independence,"  the  investigators 
observe,  "and  obedience  was  strictly  demanded," 

But  this  type  of  family  fell  upon  hard  times  in  America,  where,  because 
of  the  problems  of  the  new  environment,  achievement  came  to  be  valued 

167 


and  patriarchal  authority  was  challenged.  The  major  concern  of  the  Texan 
parent,  in  contiast  to  that  of  the  Elizabethan,  is  that  the  child  may  be 
excessively  dependent. 

This  difference  between  the  Elizabethan  and  the  Texan  families  js 
apparently  not  unique.  In  a  sample  of  30  cultmes,  the  median  age  for  the 
beginning  of  independence  haining-as  judged  by  the  tune  when  thcie  is 
reduced  contact  between  mothei  and  child-was  found  in  nuclear  house- 
holds to  be  18  months;  for  all  other  households  it  is  30  months.  One  of 
the  factors  believed  to  figure  most  importantly  in  independence  training  is 
time  of  weaning.  A  survey  of  the  infoimation  about  52  societies  the  world 
over  found  that  weaning  began  at  less  than  a  year  in  only  two,  the 
Chamarros  and  the  Marquesans,  of  the  South  Pacific.  Among  the  Texan 
mothers  studied,  the  average  age  was  less  than  9  months  (The  Zuni 
mothers  weaned  at  the  age  of  2  or  3;  the  Mormon  mothers,  earlier  than 
the  Zuni  but  later  than  the  Texan.)  With  respect  to  child-ieaiing  practices 
that  promote  a  strong  drive  for  success,  the  invcstigatois  report,  these 
Texan  homesteaders  are  extreme. 

John  Whiting  and  his  co-workers  consider  the  Mormons  an  especially 
interesting  case.  Historically  these  people  had  retained  the  patriarchal 
features  of  the  Elizabethan  family  but  adopted  polygyny  and  the  mother- 
child  household.  Closely  correlated  with  polygyny  the  world  over  is  the 
post  patrum  sex  taboo,  and  this  was  so  with  the  Mormons.  Although 
definite  evidence  is  lacking*  the  anthropologists  think  the  Moimons 
probably  adopted  another  featuie  usually  found  in  polygynous 
mother-child  households,  the  practice  of  mother  and  infant  sleeping  in  the 
same  bed. 

Under  such  conditions,  Whiting  and  other  authorities  have  theorized  in 
recent  years,  the  mother  unconsciously  redirects  her  sexual  interest 
toward  her  child  during  the  period  of  the  post  parlum  taboo.  Then,  in 
compensation,  and  in  line  with  the  universal  taboo  against  incest,  she  joins 
with  the  father  in  a  stiong  effort  to  control  the  boy's  sexual  impulses 
during  childhood  and  adolescence. 

If  this  is  so,  then  sex  training  should  be  most  severe  in  societies  where 
mother  and  child  sleep  together.  To  test  this  hypothesis,  18  cultures  weic 
surveyed.  Ten  of  these  were  found  to  be  above  the  median  in  the  severity 
with  which  sex  behavior  is  punished  in  later  childhood,  and  eight  of  these 
ten  have  the  exclusive  mother-child  sleeping  arrangement.  In  the  eight 
cases  where  the  father  and  mother  sleep  together  and  the  infant  sleeps 
elsewhere,  sex  training  was  found  to  be  severe  in  only  two, 

The  Mormons,  of  course,  when  this  study  was  made,  had  long  since 

id  the  standard  American  independent  family  structure  and  nuclear 

told.  But  some  of  the  older  practices  still  held:  notably,  the  research 

renorts,  the  mothers  had  a  warm,  seductive  telationship  with  their 

r  worked  to  severely  control  sexual  impulses  and  behavior  in 

"nd  adolescence).  The  dominant  value  in  this  culture  was 

Harmony  in  the  Zuni  culture,  and  success  in  the  Texan. 

howed  up  when  children  were  asked  what  they  most 

Texas  children  most  wanted  to  be  successful,  and  the 


Mormon  to  be  good,  kind,  or  happy.  The  most  popular  Zuni  choice  was  to 
be  a  man  or  woman,  or,  simply,  a  Zuni. 

However,  the  investigators  thought  that  the  Mormons*  change  in  social 
stiucture  should  be  accompanied  by  a  shift  from  virtue  to  success  as  the 
dominant  value  and  along  with  this,  more  emphasis  on  independence  and 
less  on  the  control  of  sex.  Considerable  evidence  was  found  that  the 
Mormons  studied  in  New  Mexico  were  indeed  moving  in  this  direction: 
one-third  of  the  sample  could  not  be  distinguished  from  the  Texans  in 
their  child -i  earing  practices.  The  investigators  predict  that  "in  another 
generation  the  Moimon  and  Texan  family  structure  and  value  system  will 
be  indistinguishable." 

Monogamy,  Economic  Progress,  Sin,  and  Guilt 

In  John  Whiting's  view,  as  the  preceding  section  may  have  made  clear, 
the  association  between  monogamous  societies  and  material  progress  is 
not  happenstance  but  rooted  in  chilcl-rcaiing  practice.  A  new  baby  is 
taken  into  bed  with  his  parents,  and  soonei  01  later  the  father  says:  "Why 
don't  you  wean  that  baby  and  get  him  started?"  So  there  is  an  early 
pressure  upon  the  child  toward  growing  up,  Whiting  remarks,  and  this 
leads  to  a  strong  prcssuie  toward  achievement.  In  the  polygynous  society, 
on  the  othci  hand,  there  is  no  reason  for  the  child  to  be  weaned  early. 
And,  being  in  a  happy  state,  why  should  he  struggle  for  achievement? 
Contrary  to  a  common  notion,  the  relative  lack  of  economic  progress  in 
polygynous  societies—which  include  the  great  majority  of  those  in  Africa 
and  many  of  those  in  the  Middle  East— has  nothing  to  do  with  the  cost  of 
multiple  wives,  cither  in  the  bride  price  paid  to  acquire  them  or  in  the 
time,  energy,  or  substance  spent  to  keep  them.  "In  polygynous  societies, 
by  and  large,"  says  Whiting,  "wives  are  an  economic  asset,  and  a  man  can 
hardly  afford  not  to  have  more  than  one." 

Children  in  monogamous,  nuclear-household  societies,  as  compaied 
with  those  in  polygynous  societies,  tend  to  grow  up  not  only  with 
different  ideas  about  the  value  of  material  progress  but  also,  Whiting 
believes,  with  a  different  type  of  superego,  or  conscience.  As  the  investi- 
gator explains  his  idea,  a  man's  role  calls  for  him  to  be  strict  and  un- 
forgiving; a  man  says:  "A  rule  is  a  rule."  But  a  woman's  rule  calls  for  her 
to  be  aware  of  contingencies  and  to  exercise  forgiveness;  a  woman  says: 
"It  all  depends."  So  the  male  conscience  is  more  strict  than  the  female, 
and  the  child  who  acquires  his  conscience  primarily  through  identification 
with  a  male  finds  it  less  easy  to  forgive  himself  than  the  child  who 
acquires  it  primarily  through  identification  with  a  female.  The  child  whose 
primary  identification  has  been  with  his  father  should,  therefore,  have 
more  guilt  feelings  than  the  other  child. 

If  this  is  so,  Whiting  goes  on,  people  in  monogamous  societies,  where 
the  father  plays  a  much  stronger  role  early  in  a  child's  life,  should  have 
stronger  guilt  feelings  than  those  in  polygynous  societies.  To  test  this  idea, 
the  investigator  grouped  28  societies  by  household  structure  and  rated 
each  one  on  an  index  of  guilt,  which  was  the  extent  to  which  a  sick  person 

169 


blames  himself  for  his  sickness.  Whiting  and  Child  had  developed  the 
index  years  before  because  self-iecrimination  as  a  response  to  illness 
seemed  "a  probably  useful  index  of  the  degree  to  which  guilt  feelings  aic 
strong  and  widely  generalized." 

The  results  were  as  predicted:  Most  monogamous  societies  rated  high 
on  this  index  of  guilt;  most  polygynous  societies  rated  low.  Further,  (he 
proportion  of  monogamous  societies  having  high  guilt  levels  was  great ei 
among  those  with  nuclear  household  stiuctuie  than  among  those  with 
extended  household  structure,  as  might  be  expected  because  in  the  latter  a 
child  is  likely  to  have  a  greater  opportunity  to  identify  with  women.  And 
among  polygynous  societies,  the  proportion  was  higher  where  the  fathei 
lived  with  his  wives  and  childien  in  the  same  household  than  wheie 
mother  and  children  lived  by  themselves. 

In  monogamous  societies,  then,  as  Whiting  sees  it,  social  contiol  is 
achieved  largely  through  the  development  of  a  strong-or  male- 
conscience,  which  gives  one  a  sense  of  guilt  and  a  readiness  to  accept 
responsibility  for  one's  actions.  But  throughout  the  world  he  finds  two 
other  independent  systems  at  work  to  keep  behavior  within  bounds.  One 
is  sorcery,  whose  believers  ate  convinced  that  antisocial  behavior  will  be 
punished  by  the  magical  power  of  otheis.  Another  is  a  sense  of  sin,  or  the 
belief  that  badness  will  be  punished  by  a  supernatural  power. 

As  foi  belief  in  sorcery,  the  investigator  finds  that  it  tends  to  occur  in 
societies  where  mother  and  child  sleep  logethei,  and  the  father  elsewhere, 
and  where  there  is  severe  punishment  for  sexual  behavior  in  childhood. 
This  combination  of  circumstances,  he  believes,  produces  anxiety  about 
sexual  impulses,  and  the  anxiety  leads  to  a  paranoid  fear-very  apparent  in 
societies  having  a  strong  belief  in  sorcery -of  retaliation  from  other 
humans. 

As  for  a  sense  of  sin,  Whiting  says  that  "the  gods  seem  to  reflect  the 
parental  treatment  of  childien,"  He  points  to  cross-cultural  surveys  by 
himself  and  others  indicating  that  where  children  arc  relatively  neglected 
during  infancy-by  not  being  fed  as  soon  as  hungry,  for  example-and 
punished  seveiely  for  aggression  in  later  yems,  the  society  typically  dreads 
punishment  by  gods  or  ghosts;  where  the  parents  are  more  benevolent,  the 
gods  tend  to  be  also. 

Whiting  is  speaking  in  general  terms.  He  believes  that  more  than  one  of 
the  systems-paranoid  fear  of  retaliation  by  humans,  dread  of  punishment 
by  the  gods,  and  a  sense  of  guilt  and  personal  responsibility-can  act  on 
one  individual  at  the  same  time.  But  he  also  believes  that  one  or  another 
system  will  tend  to  be  emphasized  in  a  given  culture,  depending  upon  the 
child-rearing  practices  of  that  culture. 


An  Inquiry  Into  the  Origin  of  Polygyny 

Some  of  the  basic  determinants  of  personality  appear  to  be  related  to 
the  household  structure,  and  the  Whitings  think  that  the  household  struc- 
ture in  turn-as  suggested  in  the  examples  of  the  Zuni,  the  Mormons,  and 

170 


the  Texan  homesteaders— is  related  to  the  environment.  Recently  John 
Whiting  found  himself  trying  to  demonstrate  this  in  the  matter  of 
polygyny,  an  inquiry  beginning  with  the  chance  observation  at  Harvard's 
Laboratory  of  Human  Development  that  societies  practicing  circumcision, 
generally  as  a  part  of  male  initiation  rites,  aie  not  evenly  distributed.  Most 
of  them  are  found  in  the  tropics,  but  the  records  show  none  for  the 
tropical  regions  of  South  America. 

As  noted  earlier,  male  initiation  rites  occur  most  frequently  in  societies 
where  mother  and  child  sleep  together  and  where  a  boy,  therefore,  accord- 
ing to  the  status-envy  theory,  may  acquire  a  primary  feminine  identifica- 
tion. In  turn,  the  mother-child  sleeping  arrangement  occurs  most  often  in 
societies  with  a  long  post  partum  sex  taboo,  and  this  taboo  tends  to  be 
associated  with  polygyny.  It  leads  to  polygyny,  Whiting  suspects,  rather 
than  the  other  way  around.  But  what  leads  to  the  taboo? 

The  tribesmen  themselves  generally  explain  that  if  a  lactating  woman 
has  sexual  intercourse,  her  milk  will  become  sour  or  thin  and  her  baby  will 
get  sick.  The  real  explanation,  Whiting  conjectures,  lies  in  the  musing 
mother's  conscious  or  unconscious  dread  that  she  will  conceive  again  and 
thereby  endanger  the  life  of  the  child  she  is  nursing.  There  would  be  good 
reason  for  this  dread  if  the  mother  was  on  such  a  poor  diet  that  pregnancy 
would  reduce  the  already  low  protein  value  of  her  milk  below  the  danger 
point. 

Combing  through  the  records  of  diets  the  world  over,  Whiting  found 
that  the  long  sex  taboo  was  found  most  frequently  among  societies  with  a 
diet  low  in  protein,  and  that  these  low-protein  societies  were  found  most 
frequently  in  the  rainy  tropics,  where  the  climate  is  conducive  to  the 
growing  of  low-protein  root  and  fruit  crops.  The  investigate!  assumes  that 
a  diet  based  largely  upon  such  crops  probably  leads  to  a  high  incidence  of 
a  protein  deficiency  disease,  kwashiorkor.  Aware  of  this,  at  some  level  of 
consciousness,  a  mother  avoids  getting  pregnant  while  she  is  lactating.  And 
when  she  avoids  it  by  abstinence,  the  husband  is  led  to  seek  another  wife. 
Thus  Whiting— while  emphasizing  that  his  assessments  of  climate, 
nutrition,  and  health  in  many  cases  had  to  be  crude-traces  a  connection 
between  polygyny  and  climate.  As  to  the  polygynous  societies  in  the 
South  American  tropics,  he  suggests  that  many  of  them  practice  abortion 
rather  than  abstinence  as  a  means  of  child-spacing. 


The  Effect  of  Chores  Upon  Character 

Analyzing  the  reports  of  the  fieldworkers  in  the  six-culture  study, 
Beatrice  Whiting  finds  fiuther  evidence  to  support  her  husband's  thesis 
that  child-rearing  practices,  and  therefore  personality,  depend  to  an 
important  extent  upon  certain  fundamental  characteristics  of  a  society. 

One  of  the  important  variables,  the  analysis  suggests,  is  the  kind  of 
tasks  a  child  is  expected  to  do.  The  most  responsible,  from  the  standpoint 
of  recognizing  that  something  needed  to  be  done  and  doing  something, 
were  those  who  were  expected  to  take  care  of  their  younger  brothers  and 

171 


sisters  or  of  cousins.  This  babysitting  choic  seemed  to  be  related,  in  turn, 
to  the  amount  of  woik  a  woman  was  supposed  to  do  outside  the  house. 
The  highest  ratings  for  responsibility  went  to  the  Gusii  children  of 
Nyansongo,  in  Kenya,  and  the  Mixtecan  children  of  Juxtlahuaca,  Mexico. 
And  in  both  these  cultures  the  women  had  to  be  away  from  the  house 
much  of  the  day. 

The  children  given  the  lowest  tatings  for  responsibility  were  those  of 
Khalapur,  India,  where  baby  tending  was  a  common  chore  but  the 
mothers  were  always  home,  and  Orchard  Town  in  New  England,  where  the 
commonest  chores  were  household  tasks  and  the  mothers  did  not  ordinari- 
ly work  outside  the  home  while  the  childien  were  young. 

No  matter  what  the  culture,  ghls  were  scored  higher  on  responsibility 
and  muturance  than  boys.  If  a  young  child  was  poking  a  knife  at  another 
youngstei,  for  example,  or  needed  help  in  ciossmga  road,  it  was  an  oldci 
sister  who  was  moie  likely  than  an  older  brother  to  notice  what  was  going 
on  and  take  action.  Such  responsibility  is  a  matter  of  training,  Beatrice 
Whiting  thinks,  and  has  a  lifelong  influence,  Specifically,  in  the  six-culture 
study,  it  was  found  that  children  who  weie  assigned  the  caie  of  younger 
brothers  and  sisters  at  an  early  age  were  more  nurturant  and  icsponsiblc 
about  all  younger  children.  Since  girls  are  assigned  this  task  moic 
frequently  than  boys,  it  is  not  surprising  that  they  score  higher  in  both 
iiurturance  and  responsibility. 

This  investigator  finds  other  behavioral  differences  between  girls  and 
boys  that  hold  true  for  all  six  cultures  and  can  probably  be  considered 
univeisal  sex  differences.  Boys  are  more  aggressive;  in  spite  of  radical 
differences  in  the  way  they  are  brought  up,  the  boys  of  all  the  cultures 
strike  other  persons  more  frequently  and  engage  in  moie  rough  play  than 
the  girls.  They  are  also  more  mobile.  In  the  six  cultures,  little  boys  wander 
farther  from  home  than  little  girls,  and  older  boys  stay  away  fioin  home 
much  more  dining  the  day  than  older  girls.  Mrs.  Whiting  is  not  sine 
whether  or  not  the  higher  mobility  of  boys  may  be  ascribed  to  differences 
in  the  way  paients  treat  the  two  sexes.  In  all  six  cultures,  the  girls  are 
assigned  more  household  chores,  so  the  girls  must  spend  more  time  at 
home.  Still,  little  boys  may  just  naturally  be  more  difficult  to  keep  boxed 
in. 

Differences  in  the  chores  assigned  to  the  children  may  result  not  only  in 
some  of  the  peisonality  diffeiences  between  the  sexes,  Beatrice  Whiting 
believes,  but  also  in  some  of  the  differences  in  how  societies  view  the 
supernatural.  Her  reasoning  goes  like  this,  If  the  chores  involve  for  the 
most  part  the  care  of  children  or  animals,  the  consequences  of  failure  will 
be  immediate  and  obvious.  If  a  baby  is  left  untended,  for  example,  he  may 
put  noxious  things  in  his  mouth  or  fall  and  hurt  himself;  if  cattle  are  left 
untended,  they  may  get  into  the  corn  and  eat  an  important  part  of  the 
coming  year's  food.  In  contrast,  the  importance  of  getting  the  dishes 
washed  or  the  house  cleaned  is  less  clear  because  results  of  failure  are  less 
dramatic.  Household  tasks  must  seem  arbitrary  as  compared  with  baby- 
sitting or  cattle  tending,  and  schoolwork-whose  importance  must  be 
taken  largely  on  faith-must  seem  even  more  arbitrary.  Mrs.  Whiting 

J72 


believes  that  a  child's  view  of  the  tasks  and  of  the  rewards  and  punishment 
associated  with  it  will  be  generalized  to  his  view  of  the  nature  of  the  world 
order,  and  this  world  view  will  be  leflected  in  religious  beliefs.  Con- 
sequently, if  children  perceive  their  tasks  as  arbitrary,  with  major  leward 
or  punishment  absent  01  remote,  as  in  the  case  of  schoolwork,  the 
membeis  of  the  society  will  tend  to  believe  in  a  future  life  wheie  rewaids 
and  punishments  are  handed  out  for  the  deeds  clone  01  not  during  this 
one.  But  if  children  know  that  failure  to  perform  a  task  will  be  punished 
at  once,  the  society  is  likely  to  believe  that  punishment  by  supernatural 
beings  is  meted  out  now  rather  than  in  a  future  life. 

Two  surveys  of  cultures  believing  that  supernatural  beings  are 
concerned  with  the  moral  behavior  of  man  give  results  viewed  by  the 
investigator  as  supporting  these  ideas.  The  fust  suivey  dealt  with  the 
effects  of  schools.  Five  of  the  26  societies  in  this  sample  have  schools,  and 
all  five  believe  in  a  future  life  where  rewards  and  punishments  aie  handed 
out  for  behavior  in  the  present  life.  Of  the  21  societies  without  schools, 
14  do  not  believe  in  punishment  in  the  futuio  life.  The  second  survey 
dealt  with  the  effect  of  cattle  raising.  Thirteen  of  the  36  cultures  in  the 
sample  are  cattle-iaising  societies,  and  1 2  of  these  believe  that  punishment 
by  the  supernatural  is  immediate.  Half  of  the  22  societies  that  do  not  raise 
cattle  believe  that  punishment  by  the  supernatural  comes  now;  the  other 
half,  in  a  future  life.  "But  ceitainly  when  we  aie  dealing  with  such 
complex  subjects,"  John  Whiting  remarks,  "moie  factors  are  involved  than 
the  ones  we've  been  chiefly  concerned  with." 

Beatiice  Whiting  thinks  there  may  well  be  a  critical  age -from  about  3 
to  about  8-for  the  influence  of  task  assignment.  Her  own  research  has 
dealt  with  children  under  10.  Older  children  have  been  inculcated  with 
such  values  as  cleanliness  and  orderliness,  she  observes,  and  they  are  better 
able  to  understand  future  goals;  so  household  tasks  and  schoolwork  will 
seem  less  arbitrary. 

If  certain  facets  of  personality  arc  indeed  related  to  task  assignment, 
Mis.  Whiting  points  out,  one  should  expect  to  find  wide  personality 
differences  within  a  society,  particularly  one  like  ours  where  the  assign- 
ments show  wide  variations.  Children  who  grow  up  in  large  families  and 
have  to  help  care  for  younger  children;  children  whose  tasks  are  obviously 
related  to  the  economic  welfare  of  the  family,  as  in  families  with  farms  or 
grocery  stores;  and  children  in  households  where  the  mother  works  and 
where  the  children's  failure  to  do  the  household  chores  results  in  chaos- 
all  these  should  be  diffeicnt  from  children  whose  chores  seem  arbitiary 
and  are  frequently  left  undone  and  where  such  failure  often  goes  un- 
punished, as  was  the  case  with  the  youngsteis  of  Orchard  Town. 


The  Relation  Between  Stress  During  Infancy  and  Adult  Characteristics 

Laboratory  studies  have  shown  that  animals,  particularly  rats,  subjected 
to  an  unusual  amount  of  stimulation  during  infancy  grow  up  better  able 
to  cope  with  stress,  whether  physiological  or  psychological,  and  to  be 

173 


significantly  largei ,  too.  The  animals  develop  faster  and  they  learn  fastei. 
John  Whiting  and  several  members  of  his  group-in  particular,  Thomas 
K.  Landauer,  now  at  Stanford,  and  Shulamith  M.  Guilders,  now  at  Bar- 
Han  Univeisity,  Israel-have  been  much  interested  in  learning  whether  or 
not  the  same  thing  applies  to  human  beings,  and  why.  If  so,  the  implica- 
tions might  be  tiemendous.  The  findings  might  even  point  to  a  way  of 
strengthening  psychological  health  for  a  lifetime  through  a  simple 
procedure  during  infancy. 

The  inquiry  began  as  the  narrowest  of  sidelines  to  the  Harvard  group's 
main  efforts.  It  was  undertaken  because  the  chance  to  compare  laboratory 
findings  with  the  findings  of  "natural  experiments,"  as  contained  in  the 
reports  on  a  number  of  cultuies  known  to  expose  infants  to  certain  stress- 
ful practices,  seemed  too  good  to  pass  up. 

The  stimulation  found  effective  in  animals  has  included  exposure  to 
cold  and  to  electric  shock,  painful  manipulations,  and  supposedly  gentle 
handling—lifting  the  infant  from  the  cage,  stroking  it  foi  a  few  minutes, 
and  then  returning  it.  Apparently  all  such  stimulation  has  a  lasting  effect 
on  the  endocrine  gland  system.  Landauer  and  Whiting,  leafing  through  the 
material  on  scores  of  societies,  noted  many  practices  that  might  be 
considered  stressful  to  the  human  infant-among  them,  exposure  to 
extreme  heat  or  cold,  the  administration  of  emetics  or  enemas,  scraping 
the  skin  with  a  shell  or  other  sharp  object,  and  tight  swaddling.  Then  they 
compared  the  average  height  of  adults  in  the  societies  engaging  in  these 
practices  with  that  of  adults  in  other  societies. 

Significant  differences  were  found  when  children  under  the  age  of  2 
were  exposed  to  either  one  of  two  main  classes  of  stress:  (1)  Piercing, 
which  includes  circumcision,  inoculation,  and  piercing  the  nose,  lips,  or 
ears  to  receive  an  ornament,  and  (2)  molding,  which  includes  shaping  the 
head  and  stretching  the  arms  or  legs,  usually  for  cosmetic  purposes.  Out  of 
66  societies,  the  men  in  those  that  practiced  either  molding  or  piercing 
were  more  than  two  inches  taller  on  the  average  than  the  men  in  the  other 
societies.  This  relationship  between  infant  stress  and  adult  statute  was 
found  in  every  major  geographical  legion  and  apparently  was  not 
influenced  by  either  diet  or  climate.  It  held  true  for  both  women  and 
men. 

Further  evidence  comes  from  comparing  two  groups  of  individuals  in 

the  Pels  Growth  Study,  which  was  begun  in  the  late  1920's  by  the  Pels 

Research  Institute,  Yellow  Springs,  Ohio,  and  is  continuing.  The  aim  is  to 

learn  what  factors  have  helped  shape  the  physiological  and  psychological 

characteristics  of  the  150  persons  being  studied.  When  the  children  were 

being  selected  for  this  work,  John  Whiting  notes,  the  practice  of  early 

inoculation-before  the  age  of  2-was  just  being  introduced.  So,  as  it 

ha^ru^ed,  some  of  the  children  in  the  Pels  group  were  inoculated  eaily; 

^ot  till  they  were  much  closer  to  school  age.  The  Harvard  team 

Uyzed  the  records  of  these  two  groups  and  found  that  the 

vho  had  been  inoculated  before  the  age  of  2  are  now,  as 

icantly  taller  than  would  have  been  predicted  from  informa- 

tature  of  their  parents. 

1/4 


Shulamith  Guilders  then  took  another  look  at  the  reports  of  the  labora- 
tory experiments  with  animals  and  noted  thai  in  every  case,  in  ordei  for 
the  stress,  or  stimulation,  to  be  applied,  the  infant  animal  had  been 
separated  from  its  mother.  Perhaps,  she  reasoned,  the  crucial  factor  was  to 
be  found  in  this  separation.  For  each  of  75  societies  scattered  over  the 
world  she  worked  out  a  separation  score  on  the  basis  of  information  about 
the  baby  and  its  mother  during  the  first  2  weeks  after  birth-whether  or 
not,  for  example,  the  baby  was  nursed  by  the  mother,  slept  with  his 
mother,  and  was  extensively  handled  by  people  other  than  his  mother. 

Dr.  Guilders'  hunch  proved  out.  In  societies  with  high  separation  scores, 
meaning  that  mother  and  infant  were  separated  comparatively  often 
during  the  first  2  weeks,  the  average  height  of  adult  males  was  65.8  inches; 
in  the  other  societies  it  was  63.7  inches.  (Males  were  used  because  the 
figures  for  them  were  more  often  available,  but  much  the  same  difference 
was  found  for  females.)  As  in  the  first  suivey,  this  difference  was 
statistically  significant  at  the  .001  level;  the  likelihood  that  it  had 
occurred  by  chance  was  1  in  1 ,000. 

The  correlation  between  mother-infant  separation  and  adult  height  held 
good  whether  or  not  a  society  subjected  the  young  child  to  a  physically 
painful  type  of  stress.  But  where  both  types  of  experience  occurred— 
separation  from  the  mother,  which  is  presumed  to  be  stressful,  and  either 
piercing  or  molding— the  difference  in  adult  stature  was  especially  marked. 
In  societies  whose  infants  were  not  stressed  in  either  manner  the  average 
male  height  was  62.9  inches;  it  was  65.9  inches  where  the  infants  were 
stressed  in  both  ways. 

Recent  work  elsewhere  with  rats,  the  Harvard  group  notes,  supports  the 
notion  that  periodic  separation  from  the  mother  during  early  infancy  is 
stressful.  In  this  research  the  crucial  factor  apparently  was  not  the 
separation  itself  but  the  accompanying  drop  in  body  temperature  (about  4 
degrees  C.)  when  the  infant  rats  were  exposed  to  room  temperature. 
Exposing  the  rats  to  low  temperature  in  the  presence  of  the  mother 
produced  the  same  effects  as  removing  them  from  the  mother.  Separating 
them  from  the  mother  under  conditions-in  an  incubator-designed  to 
maintain  the  body  temperature  produced  no  effect. 

Physical  stress  and  periodic  sepaiation  from  the  mother  in  early  infancy 
lead  not  only  to  taller  men  and  women,  John  Whiting  found,  but  also  to 
an  earlier  start  of  menstruation.  In  a  sample  of  50  societies,  the  average 
age  at  mcnarche  in  those  societies  whose  child-rearing  practices  include 
neither  form  of  stress  is  14  years;  where  one  of  these  forms  occur,  the 
average  is  13  years,  6  months;  where  both  forms  occur,  it  is  12  years,  9 
months.  Incidentally,  no  evidence  at  all  was  found  to  bear  out  "the  folk 
belief  that  girls  in  the  tropics  mature  early.*'  So  far  as  age  at  menstruation 
is  concerned,  neither  diet  nor  closeness  to  the  equator  could  be  found  to 
make  any  difference. 

Dr.  Guilders  now  wondered  if  there  might  not  be  a  relationship 
between  recent  increases  in  height  in  a  number  of  countries-including  the 
United  States  and  Japan-and  the  growing  tendency  over  the  last  few 
decades  to  have  babies  born  in  hospitals,  where  they  are  separated  from 

175 


their  mother  more  frequently  than  they  would  be  at  home.  She  has  been 
trying  to  answer  this  finding  among  the  Israelis  who  emigrated  from 
Yemen.  Befoie  the  move,  a  baby  was  usually  bom  at  home  and  spent  the 
first  several  weeks  with  the  mother,  on  hei  bed,  during  whicli  time  the 
mother  was  fieed  of  all  household  duties.  After  their  arrival  in  Israel, 
however,  Yemenite  women  were  gradually  prevailed  upon  to  give  birth  in 
maternity  wards,  wheie  a  baby  is  geneially  brought  to  his  mother  only  at 
4-hour  intervals  and  only  during  the  daytime.  Dr.  Guilders  has  started  a 
long-teun  study  of  300  Yemenites,  born  in  Isiael  between  1950  and  1957, 
about  half  of  them  in  maternity  centers  and  the  rest  at  home,  where  they 
were  cared  for  dining  the  first  weeks  in  the  traditional  way.  So  far  the 
investigator  has  analyzed  the  weight  data  for  the  fiist  4  years.  At  the  age 
of  1  month  the  homeborn  children  were  slightly  heaviei  than  the  hospital- 
boin,  but  the  difference  was  not  statistically  significant.  At  the  age  of  1 
year,  the  average  hospital-born  child  was  363  grams  heavier  than  the  child 
born  at  home,  and  by  the  age  of  4  this  difference  had  increased  to  1,097 
grams  or  about  2'/2  pounds.  Dr.  Gundeis  believes  she  has  ruled  out  all 
possible  reasons  for  this  highly  significant  weight  diffeience-including 
possible  differences  in  diet  or  in  mothering-except  early  stimulation 
resulting  from  periodic  mother-baby  separation  during  infancy. 

The  two  groups  aie  to  be  followed  through  the  years  and  compared  on 
a  number  of  measures.  The  hope  is  to  leatn  if  early  separation,  like  that 
customa'iy  in  most  maternity  wards,  has  psychological  as  well  as  physical 
consequences,  as  it  has  been  shown  to  have  in  lats. 

John  Whiting  intends  to  do  a  similai  study  in  the  United  States.  He 
points  out  that  some  hospitals  now  permit  mothers  to  keep  their  new 
babies  with  them  most  of  the  time,  and  he  thinks  it  will  be  possible  to 
compare  these  children  with  those  who  spend  most  of  their  first  few  days 
in  the  usual  cential  nursery.  He  expects  to  find  both  physical  and  psy- 
chological differences. 

Research  Grant  Mil  1096 
Date  of  Interviews'  Nov   1 , 1966 

References 

Burton,  R.  V.,  and  Wlritting,  J.  W.  M.  The  absent  father  iind  cross-sex  identity.  Men-ill- 

Palmer  Quart.  Behav.  Develpm.,  1961 ,  7,  2,  reprinted  as  A-277,  "The  Bobbs-Mcrrill 

Reprint  in  tlie  Social  Sciences,"  Bobbs-Mernll,  Indianapolis, 
Gunders,  Sluilarmth  M.,  and  WhHting,  J.  W.  M.  The  effects  of  periodic  separation  from 

the  mother  during  infancy  upon  growth  and  development,  Congr.  Anthrop.  & 

Ethnol,  Sci.,  1964. 
Guilders,  Shulamith  M.,  and  Whitmg,  J.  W.  M.  Separation  from  mother  during  infancy 

and  physical  growth— a  cross-cultural  study.  Unpublished  manuscript,  1966. 
Landauer,  T.  K.,  and  Whiting,  J.  W.  M.  Infantile  stimulation  and  adult  nature  of 

human  males  Amer.  Antrop.,  1964,  66,  5. 
Minturn,  Leigh,  and  Lambert,  W.  W.  Mothers  of  six  cultures:  Antecedents  of  child 

rearing.  New  York:  John  Wiley  &  Sons,  1964. 
Munroe,  R.  LM  Munroe,  Ruth  H-,  and  Whiting,  J.  W.  M.  Structure  and  sentiment; 

Evidence  from  recent  studies  of  the  couvade.  Amer.  Anthrop.  Assoc.  meeting, 

1965. 

176 


Whiting,  Beatrice   B    (Ed.),  Six  cultures'  Studies  oj  child  reanng.   New  York:  John 

Wiley  &  Sons,  1963. 
Whiting,    Beatiice    B.    Task    assignment    and    character    development.    Unpublished 

manuscript,  1962. 
Whiting,  Beatrice  B   Sex  identity  conflict  and  physical  violence:  A  comparative  study. 

A  met.  Anthiop.,  1965,67,6 
Whiting,  J.  W.  M    Socialization  piocess  and  personality    In  Francis  L    K    Hsu  (Ed  ), 

Psychological  anthropology    approaches   to  culture  and  personality    Homewood, 

111.- The  Dorsey  Press,  1951. 
Whiting,  J    W.   M    Sorcery,  sin,  and   the  superego    A  cross-cultural  study  of  some 

mechanisms  of  social  control.   In  Nebraska  Symposium  on  Motivation,   Umv    of 

Nebi.  Press,  1959. 
Whiting,  J.  W    M,  Resource,  mediation  and  learning  by  identification,  In  Ira  Iscoe  and 

H.  W.  Steven  (Eds.)  Peisonahty  development  in  childien.  Austin,  Tex.    Univ  of 

Tex.  Press,  1960 

Whiting,  J    W.   M.  Effects  of  climate  upon  certain  cultural  practices.  In  W.  Good- 
enough  (Ed.),  Explorations  in   cultural  anthropology.    New   Yoik'  McGiaw  Hill, 

1964. 
Whiting,  J.  W.  M.  Menarcheal  age  and  infant  stress  in  humans  In  F  A.  Beach  (Ed  ),  Sex 

and  Behavior   New  York.  John  Wiley  &  Sons,  1965 
Whiting,  J.  W.  M.,  Chasdi,  Eleanoi  II.,  Antonovsky,  Helen  F.,  and  Ayies,  Barbara  C. 

The   learning   of  values.   In  E.   Z.   Vogt   and   Ethul   M.  Albert  (Eds.),  People  of 

Rimrock,  a  study  of  values  in  five  cultures.  Cambndge:  Ilaivarcl  Univ  Press,  1966 
Whiting,  J.  W.   M.,  and  Whiting,  Beatrice  B.  Contributions  on  anthropology  to  the 

methods  of  studying  child  rearing.  In  P.  II.  Mussen  (Ed  ),  Handbook  of  reseat ch 

methods  in  childhood  development.  New  Yoik:  John  Wiley  &  Sons,  1960. 


77 


Investigator 

BettyeM.Caldwell,  Ph.D. 
Syracuse  University 
Syracuse,  N.Y. 

Prepared  by- 
Herbert  Yahraes 


A  Syracuse  research  team  has  studied  the  learning  characteristics  c 
children  from  1  month  of  age  to  3  years  and  hopes  to  relate  thes 
characteristics  to  certain  features  of  the  home  envnonment  and  also  to  th 
children's  mental  development.  To  prevent  the  retaidation  often  seen  i 
children  from  the  lowest  socioeconomic  level,  two  types  of  intervcntio 
are  being  tested:  an  excellent  day-care  program  beginning  when  a  child  i 
as  young  as  6  months  and  a  parent-education  program  that  includes 
moving  picture,  produced  under  the  grant,  showing  motheis  what  a  bab 
can  do  and  how  his  family  can  help  him  develop  interests  and  skills.  Earl 
results  indicate  that  about  half  the  childien  under  6  months  respond  t 
conditioning  procedures,  that  the  day-care  project  quickly  leads  to  soni 
increase  in  IQ's  and  that  IQ  increases  also  occui  among  babies  living  i 
homes  rated  high  on  a  stimulation  inventory. 

When  chitdien  are  very  young,  one  group  will  perform  much  the  sum 
as  another  on  developmental  tests,  regardless  of  social  or  racial  origii 
Beginning  somewhere  between  18  months  and  2  years,  however,  the  cuiv 
representing  the  peiformance  of  children  from  the  lowest  socioeconoini 
level  Begins  to  drop  and  fiom  then  on  these  children  as  a  group  scor 
significantly  lower  than  other  children  on  measures  of  ability  and  achieve 
ment. 

Commenting  upon  these  findings  by  a  number  of  investigators,  a  Nci 
York  State  research  team  points  out  that  evidently  something  happens,  c 
fails  to  happen,  during  a  critical  period  early  in  life  to  stunt  the  into 
lectual  development  of  disadvantaged  children.  Consequently,  they  cnU 
school  with  a  handicap  many  of  them  can  never  overcome. 
_  Why  the  difference?  What  happens  or  doesn't  happen?  How  can  th 
situation  be  changed? 

The  answers  are  being  sought  in  a  many-angled  research  program  undei 
taken  by  Dr.  Bettye  M.  Caldwell,  professor  of  child  development  an 
education  at  Syracuse  University,  and  Dr.  Julius  B.  Richmond,  dean  of  th 


*Now  at  the  Center  for  Early  Development  and  Education,  University  of  Arkonso 
tie  Rock,  Arkansas 


College  of  Medicine  and  chairman  of  the  Department  of  Pediatrics, 
Upstate  Medical  Center,  State  University  of  New  York.  If  the  answers  can 
be  found,  these  investigators  point  out,  they  can  be  used  to  develop  more 
effectively  one  of  the  Nation's  most  vital  resources,  the  intelligence  of  its 
people,  and  at  the  same  time  to  promote  mental  health  by  combating  an 
important  source  of  dissatisfaction  with  one's  self  and  with  society. 

One  bianch  of  the  program  seeks  information  on  how  the  learning 
process  develops  between  the  first  month  and  the  third  yeai  of  life  and 
how  differences  in  development  are  influenced  by  differences  in  the 
pattern  of  family  care.  Concurrently  the  investigators  are  testing  one 
proposed  means  of  preventing  deficits  in  learning  ability.  This  is  through  a 
program  of  "massive  intervention,"  as  Dr.  Caldwell  describes  it,  in  the 
form  of  excellent  caie  5  clays  a  week  in  the  research  group's  children's 
center. 

The  day-caic  pioject,  made  possible  by  a  grant  from  the  Children's 
Bureau,  diffcis  substantially  from  most  other  emichment  programs  for 
deprived  childien  in  these  respects: 

1.  The  project  sets  the  minimum  age  for  admittance  at  6  months 
instead  of  the  usual  3  or  4  years.  This  is  because  the  investigators  are 
convinced  that  sensitivity  to  enrichment  declines  with  age.  "The  vciy 
early   years   represent   a   crucial   period   for  the  prevention  of  learning 
deficits,"   they   report.    "Instead   of  devising  methods  of  reversing  the 
decline,  it  would  be  wiser  and  perhaps  more  economical  to  devise  ways  of 
blocking  the  process  of  decline  before  it  has  begun  to  alter  the  organism's 
adaptive  capacity." 

2.  The  project  offers  "programmed  care"  by  a  staff  of  teachers  and 
nurses  fai  larger,  in  proportion  to  the  number  of  children,  than  usual:  one 
adult  to  about  every  four  children.  The  goal  is  to  help  each  child  become 
as  aware  as  possible  of  the  world  around  him,  eager  to  participate  in  it, 
and  confident  that  what  he  does  will  have  some  impact  on  it. 

Preliminary  findings  suggest  that  this  particular  form  of  manipulating 
the  environment  does  influence  the  learning  process.  Approximately  30 
children  who  had  attended  the  center  for  at  least  3  months  showed  an 
average  1Q  gain  of  six  points.  When  these  childien  entered  the  program, 
they  ranged  in  age  from  15  to  32  months  and  their  IQ's  averaged  about 
J03;  on  the  rctest,  the  IQ's  averaged  about  109. 

There  is  no  strictly  comparable  control  group  al  this  time.  However,  the 
investigators  do  have  test  results  at  6,  12,  18,  and  24  months  for  23 
children,  also  from  the  lowest  sociocconomic  level,  who  did  not  par- 
ticipate in  the  day-care  program.  At  6  months,  these  children's  IQ 
averaged  almost  120.  At  each  rctest  from  then  on  it  dropped;  at  24 
months  it  was  about  100. 

Behind  the  day-care  program  lies  the  conviction  that  low-income 
families  often  do  not  provide  the  stimulation  that  even  a  very  young  child 
needs  if  he  is  to  develop  fully  his  capacities  for  perceiving  and  reasoning. 
But  there  may  be  another  approach  to  the  problem-parental  education. 
To  test  this  possiblity  the  investigators  have  made  a  moving  picture,  "How 
Babies  Learn,"  which  will  be  shown  to  some  of  the  mothers  of  the 

179 


children  being  studied  but  not  to  others  The  motheis  who  see  it  will  be 
encouraged  to  discuss  the  ideas  presented  and  to  ask  questions,  Perhaps 
these  mothers  will  then  provide  a  more  stimulating  atmosphere  foi  their 
children,  the  mvestigatois  reason,  and  if  they  do,  it  may  be  possible  to 
measure  the  results  as  the  study  proceeds.  Such  inteivention  will  be  far 
less  expensive  than  the  day-care  piogram;  of  couise,  the  question  is 
whether  or  not  it  will  be  effective. 

The  study  on  infant  learning  follows  a  child  until  he  is  at  least  3  years 
old.  It  keeps  tabs  on  his  physical  and  mental  development,  puts  him 
through  experimental  procedures  from  the  very  beginning  in  an  effort  to 
study  his  patterns  of  learning,  and  closely  observes  his  physical  and  social 
environment.  Where  this  environment  rates  high  in  stimulation  value,  a 
preliminary  analysis  shows,  a  baby's  IQ  increases  between  the  ages  of  6 
and  1 2  months;  where  the  stimulation  value  is  low,  the  baby's  IQ  is  likely 
to  drop. 

The  project  now  includes  50  families,  some  white  and  some  Negro, 
most  of  them  living  in  public  housing.  Soon,  under  the  plan,  it  will  add  50 
babies  from  families  living  in  university-operated  housing  for  mairied 
students.  The  new  families,  too,  will  have  meager  incomes,  but  their  social 
and  economic  backgrounds  and  outlooks  presumably  wili  differ  con- 
siderably from  those  of  the  families  now  in  the  study.  The  idea  is  to  look 
for  differences  both  within  and  between  the  two  gioups  on  a  num. bet  of 
matters  believed  to  affect  a  young  child's  learning  ability. 

Each  group  will  be  divided  three  ways  so  that  two  levels  of  manipula- 
tion—day-caie  and  parental  education— can  be  tested,  and  the  children  in 
these  subgroups  compared  with  those  who  are  merely  tested  and  observed. 

Studying  the  Learning  Patterns  of  Infants 

Not  all  children  aie  motivated  to  learn  by  exactly  the  same  proceduics. 
Promise  one  child  a  nickel  for  doing  something,  Dr.  Caldwell  points  oul, 
and  lie  does  it;  promise  another  child  a  nickel,  and  he  doesn't  stir.  One 
child  may  be  crushed  by  a  spanking,  another  gets  up  and  says,  "Ha-ha-ha 
-you  didn't  hurt  me."  A  mother  who  resolves  to  have  infinite  patience 
and  never  to  use  punishment  may  easily  stick  to  her  resolutions  with  a 
docile  child;  she  may  be  tempted  to  abandon  them,  however,  after  a 
different  child  for  the  third  time  has  deposited  a  bowel  movement  on  the 
living  room  or  supermarket  floor. 

Apparently,  says  the  investigator,  each  child  has  a  certain  learning 
pattern  which  results  from  the  interaction  between  his  inherited  charac- 
teristics and  the  way  he  has  been  dealt  with  since  birth  and  which  differs 
at  least  slightly  from  the  patterns  of  other  children.  To  test  this  idea,  the 
researchers  are  studying  each  child's  behavior  in  an  array  of  learning 
situations.  "Assuming  that  a  child  does  have  a  particular  pattern/'  Dr. 
Caldwell  observes,  "it  may  well  influence  what  he  does  on  developmental 
measures,  such  as  the  Cattell  Infant  Intelligence  Scale  and  the  Griffiths 
Test  of  Mental  Development,  For  example,  if  a  child  is  very  sensitive  to 

ISO 


social  reinforcements  and  happens  to  be  examined  by  someone  who  is 
friendly  and  smiling  and  full  of  encouragement,  the  child  is  likely  to  give 
his  veiy  best  performance.  But  a  child  who  regards  smiles  and  piaise  as 
almost  an  intrusion  on  his  own  cffoits  to  solve  a  problem  may  well 
respond  negatively  in  the  same  situation  and  thus  not  reveal  his  true 
capabilities," 

In  this  study  the  early  learning  situations  are  conditioning  piocedures, 
which  are  applied  foi  the  fiist  time  when  the  child  is  a  month  old  (and  has 
been  brought  to  the  well-baby  clinic  at  the  Children's  Centei).  It  is  now 
known  that  some  children  condition  dining  early  infancy  and  some  do 
not;  what  the  Syracuse  group  wants  to  find  out  is  the  relationship 
between  eaily  conditionability  and  later  learning.  It  also  wants  to  relate 
the  findings  from  the  conditioning  tests  to  such  matters  as  the  home 
environment  and  the  parents'  ideas  about  molding  a  child's  behavior. 

In  the  first  proceduic,  the  researcher  holds  a  yellow  disk  where  the 
baby  can  see  it  and  simultaneously  squirts  a  little  air  at  the  baby's  face. 
The  baby  blinks.  Aftei  a  few  trials,  some  babies  become  conditioned:  they 
have  learned  to  blink  whenever  they  see  the  yellow  disk,  whether  or  not  it 
is  accompanied  by  a  puff  of  aii.  Some  of  the  others  will  learn  to  associate 
the  two  stimuli  when  they  are  next  tested,  at  2  months. 

At  3  months,  when  sounds  begin  to  be  important,  the  psychologist  in 
charge  of  this  part  of  the  research-Stanley  Moldovan— changes  the  con- 
ditioning technique  and  uses  a  tone  to  signal  that  the  baby's  foot  will  be 
tickled.  Again,  some  babies  after  only  a  few  trials  will  flare  the  toes,  or 
cuil  them,  or  move  Ihe  foot— without  being  ticked— as  soon  as  they  hear 
the  bell.  Some  of  the  others  become  conditioned  this  way  the  second  time 
around,  when  they  arc  5  months  old,  and  some  do  not. 

Pieliminaiy  findings— concerned  with  about  35  of  the  babies  now  in  the 
study,  all  from  families  low  on  the  socioeconomic  scale-show  that: 

•  About  half  the  babies  tested  at  any  given  age  can  be  conditioned. 

•  Most  of  those  conditioned  to  respond  to  a  visual  stimulus,  the  yellow 
disk,  at  1  month  can  be  conditioned  to  respond  fit  2  months.  And  most  of 
those  conditioned  to  icspond   to  an  auditory  stimulus,  the  tone,  at  3 
months  can  be  conditioned  to  respond  at  5  months. 

«  But  there  seems  to  be  no  connection  between  a  baby's  readiness  to  be 
conditioned  to  a  visual  stimulus  at  I  and  2  months  and  his  readiness  to  be 
conditioned  to  an  auditoiy  stimulus  at  3  and  5  months. 

Among  older  children,  some  are  known  to  acquire  information  more 
readily  through  their  eyes;  othets,  through  their  cars.  The  Syracuse 
findings  may  be  an  indication  that  the  same  clivision-into  what  some 
investigators  call  "visiles"  and  "audiles"— holds  true  very  early  in  life. 

Usually  when  an  investigator  applies  conditioning  techniques  to  very 
young  childicn,  he  is  inteiestcd  mainly  in  learning  whether  children  of  a 
given  age  can  be  conditioned  in  a  given  way.  The  present  work,  though,  is 
directed  toward  learning  what  relationships  may  exist  between  early  con- 
ditionability and  later  behavior.  For  example,  will  children  who  can  be 
conditioned  early  turn  out  to  be  relatively  quick  learners  later  on?  Will 

181 


there  be  any  connection  between   early  conditionability  and  later  IQ? 
Between  eaily  condition  ability  and  socioeconomic  background? 

The  answer  to  that  question  on  the  effect  of  social  class  will  be  no,  il  is 
believed,  because  conditionability  very  early  in  life  is  probably  determined 
by  the  biological  characteristics  of  the  child  and  not  by  what  has  been 
happening  to  him.  Babies  from  middle-class  families  tested  by  Moldovan 
for  another  Upstate  Medical  Center  research  pioject  showed  much  the 
same  conditioning  pattern  as  those  in  the  most  recent  work.  By  the  time 
children  are  a  year  or  so  old,  howevei,  diffeiences  related  to  socio- 
economic  class-or,  perhaps  moie  accurately,  to  differences  in  the  patterns 
of  parental  caie-are  expected  to  appear. 

The  procedures  using  visual  and  auditory  signals  are  classical  or 
Pavlovian  conditioning,  in  which  one  stimulus  comes  to  be  associated  with 
another.  The  Syracuse  group  is  also  studying  the  children's  response  to 
operant  conditioning,  in  which  behavior  is  shaped  by  the  giving  or  with- 
holding of  rewaids  or  punishments.  Here,  too,  little  is  known  either  about 
the  factois  influencing  conditionability  in  children  or  about  a  possible 
lelationship  between  conditionability  and  later  learning. 

The  first  operant  piocedure  tries  to  influence  a  baby's  vocalization  rate 
at  the  age  of  4  months.  As  Dr.  Lois  Henning,  the  psychologist  in  charge, 
explains,  infants  between  the  ages  of  3  and  5  months  babble  a  good  deal 
even  when  no  one  is  around.  This  is  spontaneous  vocalization.  After  about 
5  months,  the  child  tends  to  limit  his  babbling  to  those  occasions  when 
there  is  someone  to  notice  it-and  to  reward  him  for  it  with  smiles  and 
talk  and  play-all  of  which  constitute,  in  the  terms  of  learning  theory, 
"social  reinforcement." 

There  is  some  evidence  that  babies  from  middle-class  families  aic 
rewarded  more  often  for  their  cooing  and  babbling  and  attempts  at  talking 
than  babies  lower  down  on  the  socioeconomic  scale.  And  this  is  one  of  the 
reasons,  it  is  theorized,  that  babies  in  the  first  group  get  a  hcadstart 
verbally  and  icach  school  age  considerably  better  equipped  to  benefit 
from  the  formal  educational  process. 

The  Syracuse  group  asks  whether  or  not  there  are  marked  differences  iit 
4  months  in  the  capacity  of  children  to  raise  their  vocalization  rate  in 
response  to  social  reinforcement.  If  differences  do  exist,  ate  they  mainly 
between  classes  or  among  individuals  regardless  of  class?  Do  the  results  at 
4  months  predict  anything  about  verbal  ability  and  learning  ability  later 
on?  If  accurate  predictions  can  be  made  about  these  abilities,  what 
happens  when  there  is  some  sort  of  intervention  early  in  the  child's  life- 
specifically,  parental  education  by  way  of  the  moving  picture  the  research 
group  has  prepared,  or  exposure  of  the  child  to  the  enriched  environment 
of  the  day-care  program? 

To  establish  a  baseline,  the  baby's  vocalizations  are  recorded  fora  few 
minutes  as  he  rests  in  a  quite  room.  The  experimenter  is  present  and  in  the 
baby's  sight  but  makes  no  response  to  his  babbling.  During  the  con- 
ditioning period,  the  investigator  smiles  at  the  baby  whenever  he  babbles 
and  gives  him  a  warm  "yes,  yes"  and  a  friendly  pat  on  the  tummy,  This  is 
the  kind  of  reinforcement,  the  research  team  believes,  that  operates  in  the 

182 


natural  environment  of  children  and  may  have  a  critical  role  in  the  de- 
velopment of  their  vocal  behavior  and  of  language. 

There  are  no  answers  yet.  Some  of  the  babies— again,  about  half-do 
increase  their  rate  of  vocalization;  the  others  show  no  response  to  the 
social  reinforcement.  The  investigators  think  that  responsiveness  may  well 
be  environmental-that  the  baby  who  responds  in  the  laboratory  situation 
is  the  one  whose  mother  talks  to  him  a  good  deal  when  he  talks,  so  he 
learns  earlier  than  the  others  to  use  vocal  behavior  to  accomplish  social 
ends. 

Whether  or  not  a  child  is  conditioned,  the  investigators  have  a  measure 
of  his  spontaneous  vocalization.  They  will  try  to  relate  this,  also,  to  his 
use  of  language  latei  on. 

Dr.  Caldwell  and  Dr.  Henning  want  to  learn,  in  addition,  if  this  type  of 
conditioning  would  be  a  useful  training  device  in  enrichment  piograms  for 
underprivileged  children.  So  they  are  using  it  with  a  few  of  the  infants  in 
the  day-care  program,  making  a  systematic  effort  to  increase  their  vocal 
behavior  in  the  belief  that  language  development  may  thereby  be 
facilitated. 

From  the  time  a  child  is  half  a  year  old,  most  of  the  early-learning 
experiments  involve  an  apparatus  that  can  be  programmed  to  give  him  one 
of  several  kinds  of  rewards  (a  bar  of  music,  a  trinket,  a  bit  of  food,  a  voice 
saying,  "uh-huh,  uh-huh,  very  good,  that  was  fine,  do  it  again")  in  return 
for  pressing  the  coirect  lever  or  pattern  of  levers,  The  leveis  can  be  dis- 
tinguished by  their  position  or  by  the  size  or  color  of  their  identifying 
symbol-for  example,  dots.  The  point  here  is  to  study  differences  in  the 
rates  of  learning  and  the  effects  of  a  given  icwarcl  and  then  to  learn 
whether  or  not  these  differences  arc  related  to  the  children's  backgrounds 
and  to  performance  on  IQ  and  other  tests. 

In  related  work,  Moldovan  recently  found  a  difference  in  the  reward- 
seeking  behavior  of  boys  and  girls  when  the  reward  was  a  toy  and  when 
there  was  a  delay  between  the  time  of  the  currect  response  and  the 
presentation  of  the  reward.  Both  boys  and  girls  would  start  out  by  ex- 
perimenting with  the  three  levers  of  the  apparatus  until  there  was  a 
payoff.  Then  the  boys  would  continue  to  experiment  until  they  had 
found  the  correct  lever.  The  girls,  on  the  other  hand,  would  try  to  follow 
the  same  pattern  that  had  led  them  to  the  reward  in  the  first  place.  Each 
girl  had  her  own  pattern— "her  own  superstitious  way"— of  tiying  to  get  at 
the  reward,  and  eventually,  through  a  series  of  modifications,  she  would 
make  the  pattern  pay  off.  The  investigator  cites  this  as  evidence  of  differ- 
ences in  the  ways  people  learn.  In  this  case  something  associated  with  the 
sex  of  the  children  made  a  difference;  in  the  work  going  on,  additional 
factors  may  show  up. 


Studying  the  Stimulation  Value  of  the  Home 

Four  times  a  year  a  public  health  nurse  on  the  staff  of  the  Children's 
Center  visits  each  family  in  the  infant  learning  study  and  evaluates  it  on 

183 


the  basis  of  Dr.  Cald well's  "Inventory  of  Home  Stimulation."  The  in- 
ventory contains  72  items,  all  of  which  grew  from  certain  assumptions  by 
Dr.  Ctildwell-based  on  her  own  experience,  on  research  by  other  investi- 
gators, and  on  expert  opinion-about  the  conditions  that  foster  a  child's 
development. 

The  development  of  a  young  child,  Dr.  Caldwell  assumes,  is  fostered 
by. 

1.  A  relatively  high  frequency  of  adult  contact  involving  a  relatively 
small  number  of  adults  (the  mother,  the  father,  and,  when  the  mother  is 
away,  one  of  not  moie  than  three  regular  substitutes). 

2.  The  provision  of  a  social  learning  environment  that  both  stimulates 
the  child  and  responds  to  him.  (For  example,  the  mother  reads  to  the 
child  at  least  three  times  a  week,  responds  to  him  verbally  when  he 
vocalizes,  tells  him  the  names  of  things  and  people,  encourages  develop- 
mental advances  such  as  waving  bye-bye  and  saying  his  name,  and  supplies 
toys  that  challenge  him  to  develop  new  skills.  She  gives  these  toys  added 
value  in  his  eyes  by  demonstrating  her  own  interest  in  them.) 

3.  An    optimal   level    of   need   gratification,  defined   as  sufficiently 
prompt  attention  to  the  child's  needs  so  that  the  young  organism  is  not 
overwhelmed,  but  not  so  prompt  or  complete  that  budding  attempts  to 
meet  his  needs  himself  are  aborted  or  extinguished. 

4.  A   positive  emotional  climate— an  interpersonal  situation  through 
which  the  child  learns  to  trust  others  and  himself.  (For  example,  during 
the  visit  of  the  public  health  nurse,  the  mother  spontaneously  praises  the 
child's  qualities  or  belutvioi,  does  not  shout  at  him  or  express  annoyance 
with  him,  caresses  him  at  least  once,  and  reports  that  no  more  than  one 
instance  of  physical  punishment  occurred  during  the  preceding  week.) 

5.  An  environment  that  contains  few  unnecessary  restrictions  on  the 
child's  early  exploratory  attempts.  (The  child  is  kept  in  playpen  01  jump 
chair  no  more  than  an  hour  a  day,  is  taken  promptly  from  his  crib  when 
he  awakens  from  a  nap,  is  not  slapped  ot  spanked  for  spilling  or  spitting 
food  or  drink.) 

6.  The  provision  of  rich  and  varied  cultutal  experiences.  (The  investi- 
gator is  interested  in  learning,  foi  example,  whether  or  not  the  child  cats 
at  least  one  meal  a  day  with  his  parents,  is  taken  into  a  grocery  store  at 
least  once  a  week,  goes  on  an  outing  with  his  family  at  least  every  other 
week,  is  taken  to  church  by  a  member  of  the  family  twice  a  month  or 
more.) 

7.  A  physical  environment  containing  modulated  amounts  and  varieties 
of  sensory  expeiience.  (For  instance,  the  house  is  not  overly  noisy  and  is 
neither  dark  nor  monotonously  decorated,  and  the  family  has  at  least  one 
pet,  one  house-plant,  and  10  books.) 

8.  Access  to  certain  kinds  of  play  materials.  For  a  child  under  1  year, 
these  include  a  cuddly  toy;  items,  like  beads  and  blocks,  that  go  in  and  oul 
of  a  icceptacte;  a  push  or  pull  toy;  a  fit-together  toy,  and  one  or  two  cloth 
or  cardboard  books.  For  a  child  between  1  and  2  years  of  age  they  include 
a  child-size  table  and  chair,  a  ride  toy  such  as  a  scooter  or  kiddy  car,  large 
blocks  or  boards,  bang  and  hammer  toys,  access  to  a  record  player  and  to 

184 


children's  records.  For  a  child  between  2  and  3  years  of  age,  they  include 
simple  wooden  01  heavy  rubber  puzzles,  medium-size  wheel  toys;  role- 
playing  toys,  such  as  those  used  in  playing  at  being  a  cowboy  or  a  mother; 
and  at  least  20  childien's  books. 

A  home's  stimulation  scoie  is  the  total  number  of  items  checked  "yes" 
by  the  nurse  as  the  result  of  her  observations  during  the  visit  and  her  talk 
with  the  mother. 

The  Home  Stimulation  Inventory,  Dr.  Caldwell  notes,  is  an  experimen- 
tal technique.  There  is  yet  no  proof  that  any  of  the  items  comprising  it  can 
influence  a  child's  development.  However,  a  recent  comparison  of  stimula- 
tion scores  and  of  changes  on  the  Cattell  Infant  Intelligence  Scale  between 
6  and  12  months  showed  that  positive  IQ  changes  had  occuned  in 
children  from  homes  earning  high  scores  and  that  negative  changes  were 
the  general  pattern  in  children  from  low-score  homes.  Seveial  years  from 
now  the  investigators  will  know  more  about  the  impact  of  this  variable. 
They  expect  to  be  able  to  say,  by  looking  back  at  what  each  child  had  in 
his  home  environment  at  various  periods,  which  items  and  groups  of  items 
are  the  most  sensitive  indicators  of  developmental  progiess  in  later  years. 


The  Effect  of  Parental  Theories  About  How  To  Influence  Behavior 

In  a  second  approach  to  assessing  the  family  environment,  the  research 
team  is  studying  each  mother's  ideas  about  the  most  effective  ways  of 
shaping  the  behavior  of  her  child.  These  ideas,  the  investigators  point  out, 
represent  a  mother's  theory  of  how  children  learn,  and  it  would  be  helpful 
to  know  whether  or  not  one  theory  has  greater  effect  than  another  on  the 
learning  ability  of  a  young  child.  It  would  be  helpful  to  know  also 
whethci  or  not  there  is  any  difference  between  social  classes  in  the 
pjevalence  of  a  given  theory. 

The  research  team  has  prepared  lists  of  behaviors  characteristic  of  many 
children-one  list  each  for  1 -year-olds,  2-year-olds,  and  3-year-olds.  Among 
the  45  behaviors  listed  for  1-ycar-olds  are,  for  example,  climbs  out  of  bed 
after  being  put  to  bed,  pokes  finger  or  object  into  an  electrical  outlet, 
imitates  words  01  sounds,  and  acts  afraid  in  the  doctor's  office.  The 
mother  is  asked  in  each  case  whether  she  would  encourage  or  discourage 
the  behavior-provided  it  made  any  difference  to  her  at  all-and  how  she 
would  go  about  doing  so. 

Dr.  Caldwell  describes  the  courses  open  to  the  mother  as: 

1.  Manipualtion  of  privileges  and  tangibles.  -The  mother  can  give  the 
child  a  cookie,  for  example,  take  him  on  a  trip,  let  him  stay  up  late-or 
she  can  refuse  to  do  so, 

2.  Manipulation  of  maternal  emotional  responses. -The  mother  can  kiss 
the  child,  smile  at  him,  thank  him,  frown  or  glare  at  him  or  become  upset, 
or  make  no  response  at  all. 

3.  Manipulation  of  child's  emotional  sm/e.-The  mother  can  enhance 
the  child's  esteem  by  praising  him,  undermine  it  by  ridicule  or  by  forcing 
an  apology,  undermine  it  through  a  scolding,  or  undermine  it  by  physical 

185 


punishment-cither  mild,  like  a  swat  on  the  bottom,  or  severe,  like  a 
spanking. 

4.  Manipulation   of  input—The    mothei    can   point   to   herself,  her 
husband,  or  another  child  as  a  model  of  behavior  for  the  child  to  imitate. 
Or  she  can  provide  a  verbal  explanation  of  why  he  should  behave  in  a 
given  manner.  Or  she  can  demonstrate  what  he  should  do. 

5.  Manipulation  of  the  environment. -The  mother   can   establish  n 
schedule  to  include,  for  instance,  putting  the  child  to  bed  at  the  same  time 
every  day.  Or  she  can  change  things-for  example,  placing  a  plant  out  of 
reach  if  the  child  picks  it  leaves-so  that  an  unwanted  behavior  cannot 
occur.  She  can  also  get  at  the  cause  of  an  unwanted  behavior  and  correct 
it, 

6.  Mandate, -The  mothei  can  tell  a  child-through  a  simple  command 
or  through  insistence  or  threat— what  to  do  or  not  to  do. 

The  Syracuse  group  thinks  that  all  these  ways  can  be  effective  but  that 
the  skillful  mother  probably  uses  Nos.  4  and  5  moie  than  the  others.  The 
investigators  expect  to  find  positive  manipulations,  such  as  granting 
privileges  and  giving  praise,  more  commonly  used  by  middle-class  than  by 
lower-class  mothers.  If  so,  Dr.  Caldwell  notes,  the  middle-class  mother 
would  have  the  support  of  learning  theory,  which  says  that  rewarding  a 
desired  response  is  a  far  moie  powerful  way  of  shaping  behavior  than 
punishing  an  undesired  one. 

"Maybe  only  50  percent,  or  even  considerably  less,  of  the  way  a  child 
develops  can  be  influenced  by  his  environment,  no  matter  what  this  en- 
vironment is  or  what  is  done  to  improve  it,"  Dr.  Caldwell  remarks.  "But 
environment  is  the  only  thing  you  can  do  something  about;  and  to  do  it  as 
effectively  as  possible,  you  have  to  understand  what  factors  in  it  are  most 
influential."  Parental  theories  about  how  to  shape  a  child's  behavior,  she 
suspects,  will  prove  to  be  among  the  important  factors. 

Some  Maternal  Behavior  Factors  That  Influence  Development 

In  a  related  study,1  the  same  investigators  have  been  concerned  with 
learning  how  a  child's  early  experiences  influence  his  social  and 
personality  development.  The  two  dozen  children  in  this  study  are  all 
from  low-income  families,  so  any  differences  reflect  differences  between 
families  rather  than  between  social  classes.  Among  the  data  now  available 
are  the  reults  of  developmental  tests  given  the  children  at  approximately 
6-month  intervals  during  the  first  2  years  of  life  and  ratings  covering  a 
broad  range  of  maternal  behavior  as  observed  at  intervals  running  from  3 
to  6  months. 

The  investigators  find  that  those  children  have  reached  a  higher  de- 
velopmental level  whose  mothers: 

I.  Gave  them  more  warmth  and  affection  (the  influence  of  these 
quaUties  was  not  clear  until  the  tests  at  18  months), 

'MH  0852,  "Infancy  Experiences  and  Early  Child  Development,"  Julius  B. 
Richmond,  M,D,,  principal  investigator. 

186 


2.  Specifically  expressed  a  desire  that  their  childien  do  well  in  school 
(the  influence  of  this  "maternal  need  for  achievement"  was  seen  at  12 
months  and  persisted). 

3.  Showed   an  ability  to  plan  for  the  care  of  their  children  and  to 
respond  adaptively  to  the  suggestions  of  child-care  authorities. 

4.  Maintained  physical  order  in  the  home. 

The  number  of  cases  is  small,  the  investigators  point  out,  but  the  find- 
ings are  what  might  have  been  expected.  Also,  they  support  the  idea  that 
the  value  of  a  given  home  as  a  child-rearing  environment  cannot  be  deter- 
mined alone  from  knowledge  of  the  family's  socioeconomic  status. 


A  Movie  To  Help  the  Child  by  Educating  His  Parents 

The  actors  in  the  moving  picture,  "How  Babies  Learn,"  aie  children 
being  studied  by  the  Syracuse  gioup,  plus  their  mothers  or  caretakers  and 
other  family  members.  They  are  unrehearsed.  The  narrator  is  Dr.  Caldwell. 
"You  would  be  amazed,"  she  says  in  discussing  the  film,  "at  how  many 
people  regard  the  first  year  of  life  as  a  vegetative  period  when  all  you  have 
to  do  is  feed  the  baby,  change  him,  keep  him  quiet,  and  let  him  sleep  well. 
The  experiences  during  the  first  year,  however,  may  well  be  the  most 
ciucial  learning  encounters  that  the  child  ever  has." 

As  the  picture  opens,  the  narrator  makes  two  main  points;  Here  are 
some  things  a  baby  does  in  his  first  year;  and  here  are  some  ways  you  can 
help  make  it  possible  for  him  to  develop  his  mind  and  his  skills, 

A  baby  can  suck,  react  to  light,  hold  on  tightly  to  something  placed  in 
his  hand,  move  his  head  when  placed  face  down,  and  make  other 
responses  to  his  environment,  the  movie  points  out.  Perhaps  his  most 
powerful  tool  in  adjusting  to  the  environment  and  adjusting  it  to  him  is 
his  ability  to  cry.  The  message  of  the  cry  is  not  very  precise,  so  the  mother 
has  to  try  something;  then  if  the  baby  doesn't  stop  crying,  she  has  to  tty 
something  else.  "If  he  does  stop,"  the  narrator  says,  "the  mother  has 
probably  interpreted  the  message  correctly.  For  the  baby,  this  makes 
possible  a  very  simple  but  important  type  of  learning.  The  baby  learns 
that  when  lie  cries,  something  happens." 

Commenting  on  this  point,  Dr.  Caldwell  says  that  the  first  step  in  a 
learning  situation  is  to  realize  that  what  one  does  makes  some  difference. 
One  of  the  few  things  a  baby  can  do  is  cry.  If  he  docs  it  and  gets  no 
response,  the  result  is  "a  step  on  the  road  to  apathy."  He  doesn't  have  the 
adult's  capacity  to  tolerate  frustration  and  delay,  and  he  soon  quits  tiying. 

Before  long,  the  narrator  goes  on,  the  baby  may  learn  that  a  smile  brings 
more  love  and  attention,  and  perhaps  accomplishes  more,  than  a  cry  ever 
could. 

As  the  baby  gets  his  eyes  under  reasonable  control,  he  begins  to  look  at 
everything  about  him.  But  gradually  he  becomes  more  particular.  The 
thing  that  seems  to  have  the  most  appeal  is  someone  else's  face,  especially 
if  that  face  looks  back. 

187 


By  6  months  of  age,  the  movie  demonstrates,  babies  are  interested  in 
almost  anything.  A  grasped  object  is  almost  certain  to  be  put  into  the 
mouth.  Holding  two  objects  is  difficult,  not  only  because  the  hands  have 
difficulty  but  also  because  the  attention  is  in  short  supply;  holding  three 
objects  is  almost  impossible.  And  when  the  mothers  hides  one  object 
under  a  pillow,  the  baby  shows  no  further  interest  in  it. 

Between  6  and  9  months,  however,  important  changes  take  place. 
Nine-month-old  twins  are  shown  sitting  up,  balancing  themselves  easily 
while  they  examine  their  environment.  They  handle  objects  and  taste 
them.  When  the  tattle  is  hidden,  the  little  girl  goes  after  it:  "She  has 
learned  from  experience  that  things  hidden  are  still  there."  Handling  two 
things  at  once  is  now  easier;  when  one  is  dropped,  the  eyes-but  not  always 
the  hands-follow  it.  The  boy  twin  crawls,  pursuing  toys  01  people  thai 
interest  him.  His  twin  has  not  yet  begun  to  crawl.  But  here  is  a  10-month 
girl  who  can  crawl  and  who  spends  much  time  pulling  herself  to  a  standing 
position.  And  here  is  an  11 -month-old  girl  who  walks  while  holding  her 
mother's  hand,  or  the  furniture,  and  can  even  grab  a  toy  while  she  is  on 
her  feet. 

A  number  of  other  instances  are  given  of  how  behavior  changes  as  the 
baby  develops.  "Perhaps  this  description  of  infant  learning  has  sounded  as 
though  it  all  occurred  automatically,  as  though  it  all  came  from  within," 
says  the  narrator.  "Nothing,  of  course,  could  be  further  from  the  facts. 
At  every  step  in  the  process  the  people  who  are  important  to  the  baby 
play  a  major  role.  From  his  parents,  his  brothers  and  sisters,  his  grand- 
parents, and  interested  friends  must  come  the  stimulation  necessary  to 
develop  fully  his  ability  to  learn.  Little  things  can  make  a  big  diffeience, 
Consider  something  as  apparently  insignificant  as  the  position  into  which 
the  baby  is  habitually   placed  during  its  waking  hours.  He  stays  put 
wherever  he  is  put  down.  If  kept  on  his  back,  and  if  no  people  are  in  sight, 
he  may  well  have  a  monotonous  view.  (The  film  shows  a  baby  who  has 
nothing  to  look  at  except  a  blank  ceiling,)  Until  he  can  turn  himself  over 
and  look  around  for  something  interesting,  his  seeing  and  looking  can  be 
given  an  assist  by  putting  him  down  on  his  stomach  part  of  the  time, 
From  this  position  he  can  practice  some  of  the  movements  he  needs  to 
master  befoie  being  able  to  crawl  or  walk  and  can  choose  what  he  wants 
to  look  at  to  some  extent.  Mother's  face  is  far  more  interesting  than  the 
ceiling.  Or  he  can  be  propped  in  a  comfortable  position  and  permitted  to 
examine  his  surroundings.  In  this  position  he  can  see  and  can  also  use  his 
hands  to  practice  reaching  and  holding. 

"During  the  first  half  year  or  so,"  the  narrator  continues,  "we  know 
that  the  baby  needs  people  and  he  needs  a  variety  of  experiences  to  help 
him  learn-  But  some  time  between  6  months  and  a  year  he  seems  to  need 
something  else:  he  needs  special  people.  Not  just  anybody  will  do 
anymore.  In  particular,  he  seems  to  need  his  mother.  For  some  reason, 
babies  reared  without  this  special  attachment  do  not  seem  to  learn  as 
well," 

188 


The  film  shows  a  10-month-old  girl  responding  uncertainly  and 
reluctantly  to  the  smile  and  outstretched  arms  of  a  stranger  But  when  the 
mother  beckons,  the  baby's  face  lights  up  and  she  crawls  joyously  to  her. 

Now  another  10-month-old  girl,  who  has  not  seen  hei  mothei  for  3 
months,  appears  on  the  screen.  She  is  thin  and  looks  frightened.  Though 
she  goes  fiom  one  stranger  to  another  with  little  hesitation,  she  does  not 
respond  enthusiastically  to  any  of  them.  She  passively  accepts  whatever 
happens.  When  a  cookie  is  handed  to  her,  she  reaches  for  it.  But  when  it 
falls  to  the  floor,  the  least  bit  out  of  reach,  she  does  not  try  to  get  it. 
When  a  toy  is  hidden,  she  shows  no  interest  in  finding  it.  She  can  stand, 
but  her  balance  is  poor.  "In  such  cases,"  says  the  nariatoi,  "if  the  separa- 
tion from  the  mother  does  not  last  too  long,  or  if  a  substitute  can  be 
arranged  who  will  give  the  baby  a  lot  of  tender,  loving  caie,  the  learning 
deficit  associated  with  prolonged  separation  of  a  baby  from  its  mother  can 
usually  be  corrected." 

The  movie  emphasizes  the  importance  of  other  people  in  the  process  of 
learning  to  talk.  Long  before  a  baby  can  learn  to  talk,  the  narratoi 
remarks,  he  must  be  talked  to—must  hear  sounds  made  in  i  elation  to 
objects  in  his  environment  and  in  relation  to  his  own  needs.  Parents  often 
say  that  older  children  will  teach  the  baby  how  to  talk,  but  this  apparently 
is  not  so.  Second  and  latei-born  children  are,  on  the  whole,  slowei  to  learn 
to  talk  than  the  firstborn.  "Fiom  the  standpoint  of  teaming  language," 
the  narrator  says,  "there  seems  to  be  no  effective  substitute  for  the  ex- 
perience of  being  talked  to  by  loving  and  attentive  adults." 

Play  materials  can  help  a  baby  to  learn  shapes  and  colois  and  to 
improve  the  coordination  between  eyes  and  hands.  But  the  materials  need 
not  be  fancy  or  expensive.  Empty  food  cartons  supply  a  variety  of  colors, 
textures,  and  shapes.  Old  magazines  have  pictures  that  encourage  a  baby 
to  improve  his  perceptual  skills  and  to  use  his  developing  powers  of 
speech.  And  nothing  could  be  better  than  mother's  pots  and  pans:  "Such 
sounds  and  tastes  and  such  a  sense  of  being  involved  with  objects  that 
have  meaning  for  the  whole  family!" 

As  the  film  shows  a  baby  playing  happily  with  pots  and  pans,  the 
narrator  continues:  "Thus  in  their  everyday  routines,  parents  can  nourish 
the  learning  careers  of  their  babies.  In  humble  but  significant  activities, 
parents  can  help  their  babies  learn  to  learn." 

The  little  things,  the  small  personal  touches,  the  narrator  emphasizes, 
are  the  ways  that  enable  a  baby  not  only  to  grow  but  also  to  thrive- 
"stopping  to  talk  to  or  smile  at  the  baby  while  working;  playfully  en- 
couraging him  to  try  new  things;  helping  him  to  achieve  new  and  more 
mature  postures;  helping  him  muster  the  courage  to  take  those  first  steps, 
and  helping  him  back  up,  and  reassuring  him  when  he  tumbles.  By  such 
participation  and  encouragement  the  parents  invite  the  baby  to  move  on 
to  a  higher  level." 

Prints  of  the  movie,  which  lias  been  praised  by  pediatricians  and  other 
child  health  workers,  are  now  in  such  demand  that  the  investigators,  in  the 
interest  of  saving  the  time  of  everyone  concerned,  have  turned  the  picture 

189 


over  to  the  New  York  University  Film  Library,  New  York  City,  for  salt 
rent  to  interested  groups. 


An  Enrichment  Experiment  With  Very  Young  Children 

All  but  one  or  two  of  the  24  youngsters  in  the  day-care  program 
also  in  the  infant-learning  study.  When  a  child  in  this  study  reaches  the  ; 
of  6  months,  he  is  given  priority  foi  admission  to  the  day  program  shoi 
a  vacancy  occur.  Those  now  in  the  program  range  in  age  from  6  months 
almost  3  years  and  are  described  by  Dr.  Caldwell  as  "extremely  higli-i 
children  for  whom  some  environmental  enrichment  is  essential  if  ilcvck 
mental  decline  is  to  be  prevented." 

As  an  example,  she  tells  about  Alberta,  who  was  a  sluggish  a 
apathetic  child  of  10  months  when  taken  into  the  day-care  project  at  ( 
urging  of  a  public  health  nurse  who  had  been  helping  the  family  for  yea 
Alberta's  mother  is  retarded  y  presumably  as  the  result  of  cerebral  unox 
or  loss  of  the  brain's  oxygen  supply,  sustained  as  a  baby  when  her  lie 
was  caught  between  the  lailings  of  her  crib.  The  mother,  who  is  i 
married,  also  has  a  6-year-old  son,  and  he,  too,  is  retarded,  although  Hit 
is  no  record  or  evidence  of  biological  defects. 

For  a  while,  Alberta's  mother  treated  her  much  like  a  doll,  cuddling  ai 
rocking  her  for  hours  on  end.  When  the  girl  was  about  3  months  ol 
however,  the  mother  seemed  to  lose  interest  and  began  keeping  her  in 
crib  in  a  darkened  room  most  of  the  day,  supposedly  to  protect  hor  fro 
the  extreme  hyperactivity  and  destructiveness  of  her  brother.  At  0  nionl 
the  public  health  nurse  reported  that  the  child  seemed  to  be  diiftu 
downward.  At  8  months,  when  the  girl  was  given  developmental  losls 
the  Children's  Center,  the  investigator  recalls,  "It  was  as  though  she  find 
veil  over  her  face  and  wanted  to  keep  the  environment  out  " 

In  this  situation  were  all  the  ingredients  for  producing  culturally  dck 
mined  mental  retardation:  a  disorganized  family,  a  dearth  of  percept  u 
and  cognitive  stimulation,  and  emotional  deprivation  abruptly  followir 
emotional  support.  And  there  was  a  strong  suspicion  that  the  brollusi 
history  had  contained  the  same  ingredients. 

"Perhaps  unfortunately,"  Dr.  Caldwell  observes,  "there  is  no  literal 
test  for  motherhood.  We  say  that  mothering  is  essential  and  imply  Hi; 
any  mothering  will  do.  We  seem  to  assume  that  anything  that  sustains  lil 
is  adequate  during  the  first  few  years,  and  we  have  been  very  timid  iwoi 
trying  to  change  the  environment  of  very  young  children.  Yet  not  n 
parents  are  qualified  to  provide  even  the  basic  essentials  of  physical  an 
psychological  care." 

In  the  case  of  older  children,  Dr.  Caldwell  continues,  society  often  ste] 
in  to  help  shape  lives.  In  fact,  to  provide  a  richer  environment  for  lonrniiif 
society  forces  all  children  over  6  years  of  age  into  an  institution,  th 
public  school  system.  But  much  of  a  child's  ability  to  learn,  insofar  as  thi 
is  influenced  by  the  environment,  seems  to  have  been  shaped  long  befor 
he  was  6.  The  IQ  differences  between  deprived  and  privileged  childrc 

190 


show  up  in  the  second  and  third  years.  So  the  time  to  provide  an  enriched 
environment,  Dr.  Caldwell  reasons,  begins  when  the  child  is  about  6 
months  old. 

The  day-care  program  at  the  Children's  Center  is  thus  "an  exercise  in 
circumvention  rather  than  in  remediation."  It  is  trying  to  prevent  the 
deceleration  in  the  rate  of  development  that  seems  to  occur  in  many 
deprived  children  very  early;  it  is  also  trying  to  maximize  each  child's 
potential. 

The  children  in  the  program  are  divided  into  three  groups:  those  from  6 
months  to  about  18  months;  those  from  about  18  months  ("when  they 
are  walking  and  showing  some  interest  in  toilet  training")  to  about  2'/a 
years;  and  those  older  than  2V&.  Under  the  original  plan  a  child  left  the 
piogram  when  he  was  3  years  old,  but  the  investigators  have  now  obtained 
financial  support-from  the  Children's  Bureau  (Child  Welfare  Research 
and  Demonstration  Grant  D—156R— enabling  them  to  keep  him  another 
year  or  two.  With  the  additional  support  they  hope  also  to  compare  the 
effectiveness  of  the  present  program  with  that  of  a  day-care  center  admit- 
ting children  no  younger  than  3  years  of  age.  A  piogram  for  infants  and 
toddlers  is  much  more  expensive  than  one  for  older  children,  Dr.  Caldwell 
points  out;  she  thinks  it  is  also  much  more  effective,  but  she  would  like 
some  hard-and-fast  evidence. 

With  the  children  in  the  youngest  group,  the  emphasis  is  on  individual 
attention.  At  least  once  a  day  for  at  least  a  few  minutes,  each  baby 
receives  the  undivided  attention  of  one  of  the  staff  members-usually  the 
same  one.  During  these  periods  the  baby  is  encouraged  to  reach  for,  go 
after,  and  handle  different  objects,  and  he  is  talked  to  and  stimulated  to 
respond.  Whenever  he  is  awake  and  not  in  his  reclining  chair  or  being  held, 
he  is  placed  on  his  stomach  in  order  to  encourage  visual  and  motor  ex- 
ploration of  the  environment.  The  teachers  talk  slowly  and  distinctly. 
They  repeat  the  names  of  objects  as  the  child  plays  with  them  and 
describe  an  activity  as  the  child  engages  in  it.  Learning  games,  similar  to 
laboratory  tests  of  visual  and  auditory  discrimination,  are  played  several 
times  a  week.  Particular  attention  is  given  to  tasks-for  example  learning 
that  a  reward  is  hidden  under  the  larger  of  two  blocks— that  help  a  child 
acquire  concepts  and  the  ability  to  think  abstractly. 

Children  in  the  older  groups  receive  similar  training  in  sensory  dis- 
crimination and  concept  formation,  capacities  in  which  children  from 
culturally  deprived  backgrounds  have  been  found  inferior.  The  children,  in 
small  groups,  are  read  to  at  least  once  each  morning  and  afternoon.  They 
are  given  magazines  and  books  and  encouraged  to  look  at  the  pictures  and 
to  repeat  the  names  of  what  they  see.  Crayons,  paint,  other  artistic  media, 
and  musical  instruments  are  made  available,  as  are  toys  that  help  a  child 
develop  an  awareness  of  color,  texture,  shape,  and  sound.  There  is  a 
well-equipped  outdoor  play  yard.  Self-initiated  activities  are  encouraged, 
for  they  help  in  the  development  of  self-confidence;  at  the  same  time  the 
children  are  expected  to  learn  to  respond  to  the  house  rules. 

In  this  environment,  where  the  adults  are  friendly  and  accepting,  and 
the  child  is  encouraged  to  be  curious,  to  make  explorations,  and  to 

191 


develop  his  abilities,  even  Alberta  has  made  decided  progress.  After  2  or  3 
weeks  she  became  much  more  responsive.  She  babbled  once  in  a  while  and 
she  learned  to  pull  heiself  up  on  chaiis,  and  then  to  walk.  Now,  after  5 
months,  she  acts  most  of  the  time  like  a  normal  child.  Something  happens 
over  the  weekends,  though,  Dr.  Caldwell  reports.  On  Friday  the  girl  is 
outgoing  and  bouncy;  on  Monday  she  is  once  again  apathetic  and  needs  ji 
warmup  period  of  a  day  or  two  before  responding  to  either  the  nursery 
school  workers  or  the  toys.  What  the  end  will  be  the  investigators  cannot 
predict.  She  responds  to  the  environment  at  the  Center  but  also  to  the  one 
at  home.  Peihaps  after  seveial  years  in  an  enriched  environment,  Dr. 
Caldwell  observes,  Alberta  and  other  such  children  will  have  developed  (lie 
intrinsic  strength  to  sustain  themselves  no  matter  what  the  home  atmos- 
phere. This  is  one  of  the  project's  majoi  questions. 


Research  Grant  :IAH1649 
Related  Grant"  MH8542 


Date  oflnteiview:  June  14, 1966 
References' 

Caldwell,  Bettye  M  The  effects  of  infant  care.  Review  of  Child  Development  Re- 
search: Volume  1.  New  York:  Russell  Sage  Foundation,  1964. 

Caldwell,  Bettye  M.  What  is  the  optimal  learning  environment  for  the  young  child' 
Presented  at  annual  meeting  Amer.  Orthopsychiat.  Assn.,  1965. 

Caldwell,  Bettye  M.,  and  Drachman,  R.  II,  Comparability  of  three  methods  of  assessing 
the  developmental  level  of  young  infants.  Pediatrics,  1964,  July. 

Caldwell,  Bettye  M.,  and  Richmond,  J.  B.  Programmed  day  caie  for  the  very  young 
child-A  preliminary  report.  /.  Marriage  and  the  Family,  1964,  November. 

CaldweH,  Bettye  M.,  and  Richmond,  J.  B,  Social  class  level  and  the  stimulation 
potential  of  the  home.  Presented  at  meeting  Amer.  Psychol.  Assn.,  1964. 


192 


Investigator 

Howard  A.  Moss,  Ph.D. 

Chief,  Section  on  Parent-Infant  Behavior 

Child  Research,  NIMH 

Prepared  by: 

Antoinette  Gattozzi 


To  explore  the  nature  of  early  experience,  a  research  group  directed  by 
Dr.  Howard  A.  Moss  is  studying  parent-infant  interaction,  especially 
mother-infant  interaction,  during  the  first  3  months  of  life.  The  ic- 
searchers  are  tracing  sequences  of  maternal  and  infant  behaviors  in  order 
to  calculate  the  probability  of  one  action  leading  to  another  and  to  tease 
out  from  the  total  complex  the  contributions  made  by  parents  and  those 
made  by  the  infant.  By  determining  the  detailed  patterning  of  the  inter- 
action, the  investigators  may  discover  how  it  relates  to  the  child's  con- 
genital characteristics  and  to  parents'  psychological  makeup,  their  early 
marital  relationship,  and  parental  expectations. 

Abundant  evidence  supports  the  concept  that  the  nature  of  early  ex- 
perience exerts  profound  effects  on  developmental  processes.  For 
example,  when  Dr.  Moss  was  working  at  the  Pels  Research  Institute  in 
Yellow  Springs,  Ohio,  he  and  a  colleague,  Dr.  Jerome  Kagan,  reported  the 
findings  of  a  child  development  study  conducted  there.  The  study  verified 
that  many  adult  behaviors  are  rooted  in  early  childhood;  moreover,  for 
certain  fundamental  dimensions  of  behavior,  earliest  influences  produce 
the  most  enduring  effects.  Ratings  of  maternal  treatment  of  children  from 
birth  to  3  years  old  were  found  to  be  more  predictive  of  later  childhood 
and  adult  behaviors  than  were  the  same  assessments  made  when  children 
were  older.  Maternal  protectivencss  up  to  the  age  of  3  years,  for  instance, 
showed  better  correlation  with  a  child's  passivity  behavior  during  the 
years  6  thiough  10  than  did  protcctiveness  assessed  when  the  child  was 
actually  in  the  age  group  6  through  10  years.  Some  maternal  behaviors,  in 
fact,  had  a  prolonged  sleeper  effect  in  that  their  influence  was  not  dis- 
cernible until  children  had  reached  adulthood. 

Drs.  Moss  and  Kagan  postulated  that  the  mother's  influence  is  most 
apparent  (and  perhaps  strongest)  during  the  first  years  of  her  infant's  life 
and  that  her  behavior  then  is  more  reflective  of  her  own  attitudes  and 
values  than  it  is  an  accommodation  to  her  child's  behavior.  As  the  child 
matures  and  asserts  his  individuality,  however,  he  tends  increasingly  to 
evoke  maternal  treatment  that  is  influenced  by  his  unique  characteristics. 

What  are  the  precise  timing  and  behavioral  pathways  of  these  dynamic 
intereffects?  How  does  the  child's  sex  affect  his  behavior  and  influence 

i 

193 


maternal  treatment?  What  are  the  attitudes  that  shape  the  mother's  treat 
ment  of  her  infant?  Dr,  Moss  and  his  associates  currently  are  addressmj 
themselves  to  such  questions,  which  bear  implications  for  child  menta 
health.  Such  basic  developmental  research  issues  as  the  evolveinent  oi 
attachment  behavioi,  the  establishment  of  the  prototypic  learning  frame 
the  relations  between  the  mother's  role  in  mediating  stimulation,  and  the 
child's  coping  style  are  only  meagerly  understood.  Normative  studies  such 
as  Dr.  Moss  is  directing  are  needed  to  explore  these  areas  so  that  we  may 
learn  how  better  to  help  troubled  children. 

Many  of  the  families  that  Dr.  Moss  is  studying  also  are  participating  in 
the  Child  Research  Branch's  overall  longitudinal  program,  which  is 
designed  to  yield  a  multidimensional  scheme  of  early  family  formation. 
"For  example,"  Dr.  Moss  explains,  "in  the  study  of  married  couples, 
emphasis  is  placed  on  the  physical  quality  of  the  relationship  and  the 
degree  of  affectional  contact.  In  the  newborn  infant  project,  special  at- 
tention is  given  to  congenital  differences  in  skin  sensitivity.  In  the  parent- 
infant  project,  we  can  attempt  to  determine  whether  children  with  low 
tactile  thresholds  seek  or  are  soothed  by  greater  physical  contact  with  the 
parents,  or  if  parents  with  certain  needs  or  experiences  have  a  greater 
proclivity  to  handle  or  caress  their  infants." 

The  principal  method  of  research  is  direct  observations,  made  in  the 
home  in  two  clusters  during  the  infant's  first  and  third  months.  Each 
cluster  consists  of  two  3-hour  and  one  8-hour  periods.  For  the  8-hour 
stretch,  the  observer  uses  a  stopwatch  and  sheets  of  paper  ruled  into 
time-sampling  forms  that  list,  separately  for  mother  and  infant,  30  dif- 
ferent behaviors.  Some  examples  of  maternal  behavior  are:  Holds  infant 
close,  feeds,  stirnulates-arouses,  imitates,  stresses  musculature.  A  few  of 
the  infant  behaviors  are:  Cries,  fusses,  sleeps,  is  awake  and  passive, 
vocalizes,  mouths.  Every  minute,  the  observer  marks  the  form  to  show 
what  mother  and  infant  are  doing  and  in  what  order.  Behaviors  were 
selected  on  the  basis  of  their  presumed  relevance  to  aspects  of  maternal 
contact  or  because  they  reflected  the  state  of  the  infant.  Other  researchers 
have  found  that  the  infant's  state,  or  level  of  arousal,  which  is  a  con- 
tinuum ranging  from  quiet  steep  to  agitated  crying,  is  an  important 
influence  on  the  nature  and  quality  of  his  experience. 

During  the  3-hour  periods,  the  observer  works  with  a  portable  keyboard 
connected  to  an  electrically  powered  event  recorder.  Each  key  represents 
one  of  the  behaviors.  As  the  keys  are  depressed,  they  activate  pens  in  the 
recorder  that  leave  ink  tracings  of  the  coded  behaviors  on  paper.  The 
resulting  yards  of  tracings  can  be  deciphered  to  produce  a  detailed  de- 
scription of  mother-infant  interaction  during  the  period.  Both  methods 
are  arduous  for  the  observer,  demanding  maximum  attention  and  con- 
centration coupled  with  an  informal,  unobstrusive  manner. 

About  1 ,000  hours  of  observation  have  been  clocked  so  far,  all  of  them 
by  Dr.  Moss.  Data  collected  during  the  3-hour  periods  from  a  sample  of  29 
mother-infant  pairs  are  now  being  analyzed,  in  part  by  programming  a 
computer  to  extract  sequences  of  action  from  the  mass  of  separate 

194 


tracings.  The  time-sampled  data  from  these  mother-infant  pairs  have  been 
compiled  and  are  the  basis  for  the  findings  discussed  below. 

Variability. -A.  strikingly  wide  range  appeared  in  the  amount  of  time 
devoted  to  various  behaviors.  Great  variability  was  evident  at  3  weeks  and 
3  months,  but  was  more  pronounced  when  the  sample  was  younger.  Dr. 
Moss  suggests  that,  for  the  infants,  later  differences  in  social  behavior  and 
learning  style-aspects  of  development  that  have  their  genesis  in  the  first 
weeks  of  life-are  both  reflected  in  and  influenced  by  these  revealed  dif- 
ferences in  behavior.  The  infant's  behavior  pattern  reflects  his  unique 
constitution  at  the  same  time  as  it  influences  his  mother's  treatment  of 
him.  The  squally,  fussy  child  provokes  a  patern  of  maternal  caretaking 
quite  different  from  that  which  the  placid,  easily  soothed  infant  elicits. 
Variabilities  shown  by  the  mother's  behaviors  similarly  work  to  differ- 
entiate interactional  patterns  and  their  developmental  products. 

Stability. -From  3  weeks  to  3  months,  marked  shifts  occuired  in  most 
behaviors.  At  3  months,  infants  were  less  irritable,  awake  longer,  and 
spent  more  time  smiling,  vocalizing,  and  looking  at  their  motheis  than 
they  had  at  the  age  of  3  weeks.  Compared  to  the  earlier  period,  mothers 
devoted  less  time  to  feeding  and  in  close  physical  contact  at  3  months,  but 
increased  their  total  attending  (nonholding)  behavior  through  social 
contacts  such  as  talking,  smiling,  imitating,  and  generally  stimulating  the 
baby.  These  shifts  attest  to  the  enormous  rapidity  of  infant  growth  and 
maturity  during  the  first  3  months  of  life  and  to  the  adjustments  mothers 
made,  in  part  in  response  to  the  changing  infant.  The  mother's  behavior 
also  changes,  it  seems  likely,  in  accordance  with  her  attitude  to  the  infant, 
whom  she  increasingly  regards  more  as  a  lovable  person  in  her  life  and  less 
an  an  animated  bundle  of  total,  demanding  responsibility. 

Sex  differences.- Mean  scores  computed  separately  for  boys  and  girls 
revealed  several  significant  differences  in  both  infant  and  maternal  be- 
haviors. These  sex  differentials  also  shifted  over  time:  At  3  months,  the 
infant  variables  showed  smaller  but  persistent  sex  differences,  and  matern- 
al vaiiables  were  not  as  differentiated  by  sex  as  they  had  been  at  3  weeks. 
The  findings  augment  evidence  from  other  studies  of  children  that,  as  Dr. 
Moss  puts  it,  "males  are  more  subject  to  inconsolable  states."  Overall, 
males  fussed  and  cried  more  and  slept  less  than  females.  Not  unexpect- 
edly, males  as  a  group  were  held  by  and  attended  to  more  by  mothers; 
mothers  also  spent  more  time  with  boys  stressing  musculature, 
and  looking  at,  talking  to,  and  stimulating  them.  With  their  mothers,  girls 
interacted  more  than  boys  only  on  such  variables  as  stimulating,  feeding, 
and  imitating.  Females  vocalized  substantially  more  than  males  at  3 
weeks,  but  not  at  3  months  when  the  sex  difference  in  this  infant  behavior 
disappeared.  Another  variable  showing  an  intriguing  sex  difference  was 
mouthing,  which  tallied  the  time  an  infant  mouthed  an  object  (such  as  bis 
fist  or  a  rattle)  other  than  while  feeding.  The  item  represents  a  level  of 
integrated  behavior.  At  3  weeks,  the  mean  was  36.8  for  boys  and  30.6  for 
girls  out  of  a  total  of  480  minutes;  at  3  months,  mean  time  close  to 
doubled  for  males  (61.2)  and  nearly  quadrupled  for  females  (1 16.2). 

195 


Sex  differences  in  infant  behavior  and  in  maternal  treatment  of  info 
pose  a  difficult  problem  of  interpretation.  Temperamental  differcn 
between  sexes  date  at  least  from  the  Garden  of  Eden,  and  explanatic 
have  been  sought  for  almost  as  long  a  time.  Probably  sex  characterist 
encode  quintessential  biological  principles.  Whatever  nature's  contributi 
may  one  day  prove  to  be,  there  is  no  question  that  societies  of  men  lit 
always  embellished  the  differences.  The  familiar  verse  below  is  part  c 
servation,  part  expectation. 


Snips  and  jsaails  and  puppy  dog  fails 

And  such  are  little  boys  made  of. 

Sugar  and  spice  and  all  things  nice 

And  such  are  little  girls  made  of. 


The  data  from  this  study  accord  with  the  findings  of  other  researchci 
that  demonstrate  the  very  early  occurrence  of  sex  differences.  The  phi 
siological  origins  and  biological  functions  of  these  differences  are  no 
compEetely  known.  There  is  some  evidence  that  male  organisms  nr 
geneially  less  viable  than  female  organisms  at  birth;  this  would  lead  to  , 
tendency  for  males  to  be  either  more  aroused  or  more  lethargic  thai 
females.  Further,  it  is  reasonable  to  assume  hormonal  differences,  perhap 
related  to  the  moi  phological  differences.  The  effects  of  the  routliw 
surgical  procedure  of  circumcision,  usually  performed  within  the  firsi 
week  of  the  male  infant's  life,  are  thought  not  to  extend  beyond  12  hours 

Parental  expectations,  cameos  of  the  enveloping  society,  overlay  UK 
infant's  biological  substrate  and  contribute  to  sex  differences  in  behavior 
In  paradigms,  parents  encourage  their  daughter  to  be  complaisant  and 
serene,  their  son  to  be  staunch  and  vigorous.  The  Pels  longitudinal  study 
discussed  earlier  yielded  evidence  that  the  child's  sex  influenced  maternal 
treatment.  Mothers  were  moderately  consistent  in  protecting  and  accelera- 
ting boys  but  not  girls,  and  much  more  consistent  in  restricting  girls  than 
boys.  Differential  effects  of  maternal  treatment  on  boys  and  girls  were 
found  also.  Mothers  who  were  highly  protective  of  a  male  infant  from 
birth  to  age  3  years  were  likely  to  foster  in  the  child  a  drive  for  intel- 
lectual mastery,  the  appearance  of  such  a  drive  in  girls  and  young  women, 
however,  was  linked  to  early  maternal  hostility. 

Parents1  modes  of  treatment  also  stem  from  conceptions  of  their  own 
sex  and  familial  roles.  Some  of  the  differences  in  treatment  emanating 
from  these  sources  were  exposed  during  a  procedure  conducted  by  Dr. 
Moss's  research  group.  Parents  were  asked  to  elicit  specific  behaviors  from 
their  7-week-old  infant.  In  trying  to  get  the  baby  to  smile  or  vocalize, 
parents  spent  more  time  with  females  and  mothers  tried  longer  than 
fathers  with  both  sexes.  Fathers  participated  longer  than  mothers  in  get- 
ting the  baby  to  grab  a  bell,  and  they  tended  to  spend  a  little  more  time 
with  male  infants  in  this  test  of  motor  skill. 


The  Cry  as  Maternal  Stimulus 

Correlations  were  computed  between  a  score  of  infant  irritability  (the 
total  time  spent  crying  and  fussing)  and  a  maternal  contact  score,  which 
was  derived  by  summing  the  time  mothers  devoted  to  holding  and  attend- 
ing infants  exclusive  of  behaviors  associated  with  feeding.  For  the  females 
of  the  sample,  the  correlation  was  positive  and  significant  at  3  weeks  and 
at  3  months,  which  is  to  say  that  these  categories  of  behavioi  occurred 
together  more  often  than  would  be  expected  by  chance.  For  the  sub- 
sample  of  males,  the  correlation  at  3  weeks  was  positive  but  not 
significant,  and  at  3  months  it  was  negative.  This  indicates  that  maternal 
contact  with  males— who  were,  as  a  gioup,  substantially  more  irritable 
than  females  as  a  group— was  somewhat  random  in  occurrence  at  3  weeks 
and,  by  the  third  month,  that  mothers  tended  to  spend  inveisely  less  time 
with  males  who  exhibited  greater  initability. 

In  terms  of  the  total  sample,  however,  correlations  were  positive  and 
significant  for  both  periods  of  time.  Such  an  association  suggests  a  causal 
relationship.  Drawing  on  these  observations  and  on  findings  from  other 
studies,  the  investigators  postulate  that  the  infant  cry  is  a  potent  stimulus 
shaping  maternal  behavior.  The  hypothesis  embodies  an  explanation  of 
the  sequence  of  events  between  mothers  and  males  as  well  as  the  rather 
different  sequence  enacted  between  mothers  and  females.  The  investi- 
gators' reasoning  may  be  conceptualized  as  follows. 

The  mother  learns  how  to  care  for  her  infant  in  a  round-the-clock 
course  of  trial  and  error.  She  regards  the  cry  as  a  signal  for  her  attention; 
her  responses  are  as  ingenious  and  varied  as  she  can  make  them.  With 
practice  and  experience  she  learns  how  best  to  respond,  and  attains  some 
degree  of  success  in  reducing  what  is  to  her  the  noxious  stimulus  of  the 
cry  and  in  gratifying  her  maternal  needs.  If  her  infant  is  a  male,  she  learns 
by  the  third  month  that  she  often  is  unable  to  quiet  him.  More  and  more 
frequently,  she  may  avoid  the  possibility  of  failure  by  not  responding  at 
all,  or  by  delaying  her  response  to  his  cry.  If  her  infant  is  a  gill,  however, 
the  mothei  learns  a  different  lesson.  Because  her  daughter  is  usually 
soothed  by  her  attention,  the  mother  feels  fairly  confident  of  her  ability 
to  succeed  and  responds  consistently  to  the  cry  with  caretaking  activity. 

This  hypothesis  is  based  on  the  correlations  obtained  between  infant 
irritability  and  maternal  contact,  and  on  the  finding  that  males  were 
considerably  more  irritable  than  females  during  both  periods  of  observa- 
tion, which  may  imply  that  males  had  not  been  as  uniformly  soothed  as 
females  by  maternal  contact. 

The  interpretation  provides  a  way  of  viewing  the  first  phases  of 
socialization,  the  gradual  but  ineluctable  process  through  which  an 
individual  is  transformed  from  a  relentlessly  egocentric  newborn  into  a 
more  or  less  cooperative  member  of  society.  In  the  beginning,  the 
investigators  suggest,  the  infant  arbitrates  the  pattern  of  interaction 
through  the  powerful  stimulus  of  the  cry.  As  time  passes  he  associates 
succor  and  comfort  with  his  mother,  and  this  increases  her  effectiveness  in 
regulating  his  behavior.  Imperceptibly,  the  two  move  toward  a  juncture 

197 


wmch  the  larger  role  in  determining  the  interaction  shifts  to  the 
mother.  At  this  point,  socialization  may  be  said  to  begin. 

If  this  is  a  valid  notion,  infants  who  are  usually  soothed  by  the  mothers' 
responses  to  their  cries  should  be  more  susceptible  to  social  learning  than 
infants  whose  mothers  do  not  answer  their  cries  consistently  or  who  are 
not  quieted  by  maternal  care.  In  the  sample  under  study,  the  first  class  of 
experience  corresponds  to  the  females  as  a  group,  and  the  second  to  the 
mate  group.  Indeed,  this  view  of  one  aspect  of  early  socialization  is  in  line 
with  results  of  other  studies  and  fits  the  common  observation  that  girls  are 
more  sociable  than  boys, 


Psychological  Factors  in  Maternal  Treatment 

Infant  irritability   is  one  important  influence  shaping  mother-infant 
infant  interaction  that  can  be  isolated  and  measured.  Parceling  out  its 
effects  may  expose  the  presence  of  other  influential  factors.  The  le- 
searchers  derived  a  measure  of  maternal  responsivity  from  the  data  by 
totaling  the  amount  of  maternal  contact  that  was  not  in  response  to 
irritability.  This  tactic  uncovered   two  things.  First>  it  was  possible  to 
classify  a  mother  as  an  over-responder  or  an  under-responder,  depending 
on  whether  her  predicted  contact  score  (based  on  her  infant's  irritability) 
was  above  or  below  her  actual  contact  score.  Second,  even  accounting  for 
greater  male  irritability,  mothers  were  found  to  have  significantly  more 
contact  with  boys  than  with  girls  on  such  variables  as  "attends"  and 
"stimulates-arouses;"  on   the  social   item   "imitates,"  girls  showed  the 
higher  mean  score.  This  finding  of  sex  differentiation  in  maternal  treat- 
ment also  was  made  when  the  same  type  of  analysis  was  done  while 
controlling  the  amount  of  time  the  infant  spent  in  sleep.  In  this  instance, 
males  showed  higher  mean  scores  on  the  variables  "stresses  musculature" 
and  "stimulates-arouses,"  and  females  scored  higher  again  on  "imitates," 
Thus,  sex  differences  in  maternal  treatment  that  stemmed  from  mothers 
were  discovered  by  controlling  the  effects  of  sleep  and  irritability,  the 
salient  aspects  of  the  infant  state. 

The  investigators  are  seeking  antecedents  that  might  link  to  this  finding 
in  the  data  they  have  gathered  from  parents.  In  addition,  parents  of  23 
infant  subjects  had  been  extensively  interviewed  long  before  their  baby's 
birth  for  the  Branch  study  of  early  marriage.  Two  variables  relating  to 
potential  parenthood  that  were  extracted  from  this  latter  material  have 
proved  germane  to  the  mother-infant  study:  "Acceptance  of  nurturant 
role"  and  "degree  baby  seen  in  positive  sense"  are  prematernal  variables 
that  may  be  predictive  of  maternal  responsivity. 

The  researchers  also  have  located  tentative  links  between  a  woman's 
maternal  behavior  and  her  childhood  and  early  family  experiences.  The 
maternal  behavioral  composites  of  affectionate  contact  and  responsivity 
appear  to  be  related  to  a  woman's  recalled  attitude  toward  the  emergence 
of  her  secondary  sex  characteristics  and  the  degree  of  identification  she 
had  with  her  father.  Further  substantive  details  like  these,  delineating 

198 


continuities  in  attitudes  that  structure  maternal  behavior,  may  be  found 
by  the  current  search  for  relationships  between  early  mairiagc  and 
mother-infant  variables.  The  investigators  are  similarly  analyzing  the  data 
collected  by  the  icsearchers  who  studied  their  infant  subjects  as  newborns; 
heie  they  are  looking  for  relationships  between  an  infant's  congenital 
behavior  and  the  behavioi  displayed  in  that  infant's  inteiaction  with 
parents.  Meanwhile,  Dr.  Moss  and  his  associates  have  begun  another  study 
with  a  larger  sample  of  parent-infant  pairs.  They  hope  to  replicate  the 
findings  of  the  first  study,  and  to  follow  sequences  of  action  with  greater 
refinement. 

Dr.  Moss  is  also  interested  in  the  mother's  role  in  mediating  stimulation 
for  her  infant.  Past  studies  have  accumulated  evidence  indicating  that 
infants  have  a  need  for  stimulation,  and  that  its  quantity  and  quality  aie 
influential  along  many  channels  of  development.  Too  much  or  too  little, 
too  intense,  chaotic,  or  monotonous  stimulation,  as  uniquely  experienced 
by  the  individual  infant,  may  have  deleterious  effects.  Clearly,  mothers 
play  a  central  role  in  balancing  stimulation  for  their  infants  as  they  arouse 
or  quiet  them  and  icgulate  their  autonomically  generated  stimulation.  In 
the  mother-infant  pahs  studied,  motheis  offered  male  infants  gi cater 
amounts  of  motor  stimulation.  At  the  same  time,  as  one  possible  inter- 
pretation of  the  data  suggests,  males  were  experiencing  heightened  levels 
of  internal  and  self-stimulation.  If  this  pattern  is  found  again  in  the  new 
study,  its  meaning  and  implications  will  wanant  further  exploration. 

Dr.  Moss  believes  that  apart  from  survival,  coping  with  stimulation  is 
the  infant's  consuming  developmental  task.  In  this  context,  a  mother's 
part  in  mediating  stimulation  is  a  fundamental  task  for  her.  Studies  of 
parent-infant  interaction  may  demonstrate  how  different  mothers  handle 
stimulation  for  their  infants,  how  differences  in  infants  affect  the  stimula- 
tion balance,  and  what  relations  exist  between  the  kind  of  stimulus 
dependency  established  and  later  developmental  configurations  of  social 
behavior  and  learning  style. 

Intmmwal:  NIMH 

Date  of  Interview:  Mar.  23,1966 

References: 

Kagan,  J,,  &  Moss,  H,  A.  The  stability  of  passive  and  dependent  behavior  from  child- 
hood through  adulthood.  Child  Develpm.,  I960,  31,  577-59  1 . 

Kagan,  J.,  &  Moss,  H.  A.  Birth  to  maturity:  A  study  in  psychological  development, 
New  York:  John  Wiley,  1962. 

Moss,  H.  A.  Methodological  issues  in  studying  mother-infant  interaction.  Amer.  J. 
Orthopsychiat,,  1965,  35  (3),  482-486. 

Moss,  H.  A.  Coping  behavior,  the  need  for  stimulation,  and  normal  development. 
Merrill-Palmer  Quart.  Behav.  Developm,,  1 965 ,  1 1 ,  1 7 1  -1 79 . 

Moss,  H.  A.  Sex,  age,  and  state  as  determinants  of  mother-infant  interaction.  Merrill- 
Palmer  Quart.  Behav.  Developm.,  in  press. 

Moss,  H.  A.,  &  Kagan,  J.  Stability  of  achievement  and  iccognition  seeking  behaviors 
from  early  childhood  through  adulthood.  J.  abnorm,  soc.  Psycho!.,  1961,  62, 
504-513. 

Moss,  H.  A.,  &  Kagan,  J.  Report  on  personality  consistency  and  change  from  the  Pels 
Longitudinal  Study.  Vita  hum.,  Basel,  1964,7,127-138. 

199 


Investigator. 

Nahman  H,  Greenberg,  M.D. 
University  of  Illinois  College  of  Medicine 
Chicago,  III. 

Prepared  bv 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Through  an  intensive  study  of  physiological  patterns  during  infancy  ami 
of  the  interaction  between  infants  and  their  mothers,  a  University  of 
Illinois  psychiatrist  is  seeking  hard  and  fast  information  about  the  origins 
of  emotional  difficulties  and  psychosomatic  illness.  The  investigator  is 
Nahman  H.  Greenberg,  M.D.,  director  of  the  Child  Development  Clinical 
and  Research  Unit  at  the  University's  College  of  Medicine,  Chicago. 

Dr.  Greenberg  points  out  that  a  baby's  development— and,  according  to 
psychoanalytic  theory,  its  emotional  health  in  later  life  as  well  is 
profoundly  affected  by  the  quality  of  its  mother's  love.  The  investigator 
wants  to  find  out  how  this  love  operates,  so  he  focuses  not  only  on  how  a 
mother  feels  or  says  she  feels  but  also  on  what  she  does.  Certain  types  of 
maternal  activity,  he  believes,  either  satisfy  or  fail  to  satisfy  certain  of  the 
infant's  biological  needs  and  may  influence  either  for  better  or  worse  the 
infant's  developing  social  relationships.  Consequently,  if  mothers'  actions 
and  infants'  responses,  and  vice  versa,  are  cataloged  and  analyzed, 
eventually  it  should  be  possible  to  relate  a  given  pattern  of  action-mid- 
response  in  infancy  to  a  given  type  of  disturbance  in  later  childhood  find 
perhaps  in  adult  life.  The  relationships  can  be  established,  of  course,  only 
by  such  long-term  studies  ns  the  investigator  has  embarked  upon. 

In  one  part  of  his  program,  Dr.  Greenberg  is  determining  how  various 
kinds  of  activity  on  the  part  of  an  infant  affect  its  physiological  state  as 
shown  by  respiration,  heart  rate,  body  motility,  and  body  tonicity.  This 
Information  will  provide  a  baseline  for  measuring  the  effect  of  llic 
mother's  behavior  upon  these  same  characteristics, 

The  babies  being  studied  this  way  include  a  so-called  normative  group, 
born  to  the  wives  of  medical  and  dental  students  at  the  university,  and  an 
institutional  group  born  to  unwed  mothers-mainly  white  girls,  of  nil 
socioeconomic  levels-and  living  in  a  foundling  home, 

200 


The  measurements  are  made  in  the  nursery  of  Dr.  Greenberg's  psycho- 
physiology  laboratory  for  at  least  an  hour  at  a  time  as  the  baby  sleeps, 
tosses,  plays,  smiles,  cries,  or  does  whatever  else  comes  naturally.  By 
pushing  a  button,  an  observer  indicates— on  the  magnetic  tape  which  is 
recording  the  measurements— the  kind  of  activity  going  on  at  a  particular 
time.  Moving  picture  cameras,  one  for  closeups  and  one  for  distant  views, 
are  available  for  filming  all  or  part  of  a  session.  At  one  point  in  each 
observation  period  the  experimenter  introduces  a  "sensory  event"  and 
notes  the  results.  This  point  occurs  when  the  baby  fusses  because  he  is 
hungry,  and  the  "sensory  event"  consists  of  attempts  to  pacify  him  in 
various  ways. 

The  ways  in  whicli  mothers  and  babies  interact  are  observed  and  filmed 
in  the  same  laboratory.  In  addition,  the  mothers  are  studied  through 
psychiatric  interviews  and  psychological  tests. 

The  institutional  group  of  babies  was  added  to  the  research  plan  so  that 
infants  who  were  all  being  reared  under  the  same  conditions  and,  of 
course,  in  the  absence  of  their  natural  mothers,  could  be  compared  with 
the  others.  Through  a  data  collection  system  at  the  foundling  home,  the 
investigator  receives  a  24-hour-a-day  record  on  tape  showing  foi  each  baby 
when  it  moved,  cried,  and  was  lifted  from  the  crib.  He  also  receives  a 
filmed  record-made  by  preset  cameras  at  various  times— of  the  activity  of 
the  aides  caring  for  the  babies. 

In  a  second  part  of  the  research,  the  investigator  is  concerned  with 
infants—and  their  mothers— in  whom  developmental  disorders  are  already 
apparent.  Several  of  these  babies  have  appeared  in  the  so-called  normative 
group;  the  others-there  are  now  45  in  all-have  been  referred  to  him  over 
a  2-year  period  from  the  university's  department  of  pediatrics  and  the 
well-baby  clinics  of  the  Infant  Welfare  Society  of  Chicago.  When  Dr. 
Greenberg  first  saw  them,  they  ranged  in  age  from  3  to  23  months.  All 
had  been  raised  in  their  own  families.  They  were  not  mentally  retarded 
nor  did  they  have  neurological  defects. 

Among  the  symptoms  of  abnormal  development  were  disorders 
described  in  general  as  hypermotility,  including  head-rolling,  body 
swaying,  and  some  forms  of  self-injury,  mainly  head-banging,  nutritional 
anemia,  pica  and  other  feeding  disorders,  and  failure  to  thrive. 

As  Dr.  Greenberg  points  out,  it  is  generally  impossible  for  an  investi- 
gator who  is  stuyding  disturbed  adolescents  or  adults  to  dig  back  and  get 
accurate  data  about  their  infancy.  But  given  an  infant  showing  a  develop- 
mental disorder,  it  may  well  be  possible  to  find  out  what  has  gone  wrong 
so  far  in  his  very  short  life,  and  then  perhaps  to  correct  it  or  at  least  to  set 
up  warning  signs  for  other  families  and  physicians. 

"The  effort  is  to  work  back  and  reconstruct  the  developmental 
history,"  Dr.  Greenberg  explains,  "and  learn  what  the  mothers  did  to  get 
something  like  body-rocking  or  head-banging,  or  eating  plaster  off  the 
wall,  or  refusing  to  take  anything  but  a  bottle  and  developing  an  anemia  as 
the  result."  As  a  major  part  of  this  effort,  the  investigator  and  his  as- 
sociates observe  and  record  the  behavior  of  mother  and  baby  during 
extended  visits  to  the  laboratory  nursery.  The  researchers  are  particularly 

201 


interested  in  the  mother-baby  relationship  at  feeding  time  and  the  bab) 
behavior  dining  what  would  ordinarily  be  a  period  of  mild  stress,  win 
the  mother  leaves  him  alone  and  then  a  stranger  comes  in. 


Preliminary  Findings 

The  rationale  of  this  research  program  and  the  findings  to  dale  ar 
presented  in  some  detail  in  the  succeeding  sections.  The  main  finding 
may  be  summarized  as  follows: 

1.  At  1  month  of  age,  the  institutional  babies  observed  during  a  pilo 
study  could  be  pacified  more  easily  than  the  others  and,  unlike  the  others 
equally  well  by  any  of  the  methods  used.  Pacification  brought  a  grcatei 
change  in  their  heart  rate.  At  2  months,  the  institutional  babies  had  gainec 
only  half  as  much  weight  as  the  others.  These  differences  seem  traceable 
to  the  amount  of  stimulation  provided  in  the  foundling  home. 

2.  On  the  average,  the  institutional  babies  were  handled~for  feeding, 
bathing,  and  all  other  purposes-only  about  90  minutes  a  day.  Never- 
theless, the  institutional  environment  is  less  uniform   than   had  been 
expected,  for  some  babies  were  found  to  get  considerably  more  attention 
than  others,  (The  differences  are  going  into  the  babies'  records,  and  an 
effort  will  be  made  to  study  their  effect.) 

3.  The  abnormal  behavior  of  the  infants  with  developmental  disorders 
appears  to  arise  from  serious  disruptions  in  the  infant-mother  relationship, 
and  these  disruptions  apparently  occur  because  the  mother  is  emotionally 
disturbed.  For  the  most  part,  the  mothers  are  considered  to  be  either 
psychotic  or  borderline.  "Commonly  there  has  been  severe  trauma  in  their 
own  lives,"  the  investigator  reports,  "and  they  identify  with  the  baby  in 
terms  of  their  own  traumatic  childhood  and  mothering." 

4.  Most  of  the  infants  with  developmental  problems  show  an  ab- 
normality  in   the    way  they   pacify   themselves.   Finger-sucking  is  un- 
common, and  many  of  the  infants  have  developed  an  aversion  to  bottles. 
To  reduce  tension,  the  investigator  reports,  these  infants  tend  to  engage  in 
hypermotility,  such  as  head-rolling  or  body-swaying. 

5.  In  their  relations  with  these  infants,  the  mothers  appear  to  be  either 
detached  and  neglectful  or  overstimulating  and  harsh.  When  the  mothers 
feed  their  babies,  they  show  little  or  no  tenderness.  Some  of  the  same 
mothers,  though,  handle  their  infants  for  inordinately  long  periods  of 
time.   Their   behavior  in   this   respect,   the  investigator  reports,  over- 
stimulates  the  infant  and  is  inappropriate  to  its  needs, 

6.  Separated  from  their  mothers,  these  babies  do  not  show  the  typical 
response  of  sobbing  or  crying.  In  the  hospital,  away  from  "psychotoxic" 
influences,  the  babies  tend  to  improve  quickly. 

7.  Most  of  the  mothers  are  resistant  to  initial  attempts  with  psy- 
chotherapy. 

8.  There  is  nothing  to  indicate  so  far  whether  or  not  the  developmental 
problems   and   the    disturbed    infant-mother    relationship    will    affect 

202 


personality  and  emotional  health  later  m  life.  The  investigator  believes 
that  they  will. 

9.  The  great  majority  of  the  infants  in  the  atypical  group  come  from 
families  at  the  lower  socioeconomic  levels.  The  investigator  hopes  to 
check  reports  that  the  pattern  of  disturbance  among  infants  from  families 
at  the  higher  levels  is  different. 

The  Interactions  of  Mother  and  Infant 

Dr.  Greenberg  defines  psychosomatic  differentiation,  his  field  of  study, 
as  the  piocess  of  growth  by  which  individual  psychophysiological  patterns 
develop.  This  process  is  most  vulnerable  to  outside  influences  at  the  stage 
of  least  differentiation,  and  this  stage— with  respect  to  the  influences  of 
motheiing-begins  at  birth.  Because  the  mother  in  large  part  creates  the 
infant's  sensory  environment,  it  is  she  who  most  strongly  influences  the 
developmental  process.  But  the  investigator  points  out  that  the  infant 
itself,  through  its  inborn  propensities,  may  influence  both  the  amount  and 
type  of  maternal  behavior.  An  infant  who  sleeps  most  of  the  day  can  be 
expected  to  influence  a  mother's  behavior  differently  from  an  infant  who 
has  been  irritable-in  the  sense  of  being  peculiarly  susceptible  to 
stimuli-since  birth. 

In  studying  an  infant's  heart  rate,  muscle  tension,  and  other  measures 
of  physiological  activity,  and  how  these  are  affected  by  the  mother,  the 
researchers  distinguish  six  basic  behavioral  states  or  degrees  of  internal 
tension:  Sleeping,  drowsy,  awake-inactive,  awake-active,  fussy,  and  crying. 
To  a  great  extent,  the  investigator  explains,  a  mother  can  regulate  these 
states  through  such  activities  as  feeding,  holding,  talking  to,  and  smiling  at 
her  baby.  A  particular  activity  by  the  mother  calls  into  use  and  helps 
develop  a  particular  sensorimotor  mechanism  in  the  infant.  In  other 
words,  mother  and  infant  are  two  psychophysiological  systems,  which 
interact  through  specific  mechanisms  of  stimulation  and  pacification.  As 
these  mechanisms  come  into  play,  there  is  a  change  in  the  degree  of  the 
infant's  inteinal  tension,  and  of  the  mother's  as  well. 

A  mother  reacts  to  the  different  behavioral  states  in  different  ways,  and 
one  mother's  reaction  to  a  given  state  may  differ  from  another's.  In  one 
case,  for  example,  Dr.  Greenberg  found  that  the  mother  tended  to  avoid 
her  baby  when  it  reached  the  crying  state,  but  when  the  baby  was  quiet, 
would  begin  talking  to  and  playing  with  it  to  the  point  of  oveistimulation. 
At  least  party  because  of  the  mother's  way  of  reacting,  the  investigator 
believes,  the  baby  developed  a  serious  nutritional  disorder  and  gave 
evidencc-for  instance,  by  showing  greater  distress  than  normal  when  the 
mother  left  the  room-of  emotional  difficulties. 

Requirements  for  Normal  Development 

As  Dr.  Greenberg  sees  it,  an  infant's  responses  to  its  mother's  stimula- 
tion have  two  functions.  In  the  first  place,  they  satisfy  needs.  Sucking,  for 

203 


example,  Is  necessary  to  satisfy  the  need  for  nutrition;  visual  SL'i 
are  necessary  if  the  visual  apparatus  is  to  mature  and  serve  thu  inl;in 
normal  needs  to  lecognize  those  close  to  him  and  then  to  exploit1  itud 
learn.  In  the  second  place,  the  infant's  responses  serve  to  pacify  ;ii 
soothe    him.    Up    to    a    point,    sucking-even   when   no   food   is  heir 
received -serves  as  a  normal  pacifier  or  a  way  of  reducing  tension.  'M 
same  is  true  of  movements  of  arms,  legs,  body. 

For  development  to  proceed  normally,  the  investigatoi  hypothesise 
the  infant's  feeding  experiences  must  be  such  that  the  organism  remnnh  i 
equilibrium.  On  the  one  hand,  the  organism  must  be  sufficiently  annm.1 
by  the  mother's  actions  to  respond  to  sensory  stimuli;  on  the  other  Itinn 
the  amount  of  tension  generated  must  stay  within  bounds.  When  tli 
equilibrium  is  upset,  a  variety  of  behavior  distortions  may  arise 
them,  insufficient  sucking,  hyperirritabilily  associated  with 
regurgitation,  a  fearful  avoidance  of  food,  and  aversion  to  dietary  clungi- 
A  mother  who  does  not  establish  a  warm  relationship  with  her  inl;m 
through  feeding,  the  investigator  goes  on,  may  attempt  to  enj>;ige  lihi 
through  visual  interactions  such  as  smiling,  or  through  excessive  I»K|> 
stimulation  in  the  form  of  rocking  and  carrying.  In  either  case,  [hi*  stimuli 
the  infant  receives  aie  probably  insufficiently  diversified  for  UK"  IUA! 
possible  development  for  his  nervous  system. 

Unable  to  cope  naturally  with  the  feeding  situation,  a  mother  may  uko 
withdraw  from  the  infant  and  avoid  any  but  the  most  necessary  conMci. 
In  this  case,  lacking  appropriate  levels  of  stimulation  from  on  (side,  (\\c 
infant  becomes  irritable  and  excited  and  may  turn  to  various  toclini(|iiis 
of  stimulating  or  pacifying  himself.  In  the  earliest  months,  Dr.  (JtviMihu'ij? 
reports,  he  may  engage  in  excessive  sucking,  particularly  of  his  hfinris 
Later  on  he  may  turn  to  body-rocking,  head-rolling,  or  other  rhythmic 
motor  activity-all  in  the  interest  of  ameliorating  tension. 

In  sum,  under  this  hypothesis,  when  the  mother's  behavior  inipumor 
overtaxes  the  normal  sensorimotor  routes  for  satisfying  needs,  the  inluut 
uses  certain  of  his  developing  functions  maladaptiveiy— that  is,  for  tension 
reduction.  As  the  individual  develops,  Dr.  Greenberg  believes^  he  will  mil- 
grow  a  particular  maladaptation  but,  if  stressful  conditions  continue  lake 
on  another.  Thus  the  infant  with  a  developmental  disorder  such  as  luMd- 
banging  may  become,  unless  his  distress  is  removed,  the  child  or  uilult 
with  a  character  problem,  a  psychosomatic  illness,  a  psychosis,  or  MUM 
other  impairment. 

The  investigator  emphasizes  that  he  has  no  evidence  that  an  inftnil  who 
bangs  his  head,  bites  his  hand,  eats  dirt,  or  shows  other  developmental 
abnormalities  is  more  likely  than  other  infants  to  be  headed  toward  severe 
emotional  difficulties  later  on.  Dr.  Greenberg  merely  hypothesizes  ehut 
tin's  is  so.  He  believes  that  only  a  longitudinal  study-one  that  follows  mi 
infant  through  the  years  rather  than  one  that  tries  to  look  back  from  l:ilcr 
life  to  infancy-can  say  for  sure. 

"Hopefully,"  he  says,  "we  may  begin  to  develop  objective  means  of 
describing  psych ophysiological  development  and  look  at  some  factors  in 
maternal  behavior  influencing  them.  We  shall  be  learning  more  about  the 

204 


characteristics  of  the  'dark  age'  of  infancy  so  that  our  notions  about 
health  and  disease  in  terms  of  this  period  of  life  will  be  based  upon 
objective  data." 


Troubled  Mothers 

The  mothers  of  the  infants  with  development  problems— and  particular- 
ly the  mothers  whose  babies  rolled  their  heads  or  manifested  disturbed 
mottlity  in  other  ways—were  found  to  have  difficulty  in  managing  their 
aggiessivc  impulses.  In  response  to  an  infant's  display  of  vigor,  Dr. 
Greenberg  xeports,  these  mothers  tended  to  show  hostility  and  at  times  to 
engage  in  almost  assaultive  activity,  Fuither,  if  a  baby  tried  to  suck  a  hand 
or  a  thumb,  the  mother  would  draw  it  away,  often  lather  roughly.  At 
feeding  time  the  mothers  tended  to  act  in  an  impersonal,  get-the-job-done 
manner  described  as  "institutional."  But  sometimes  they  went  to  the 
opposite  extreme  and  strove  to  attract  the  infant's  attention  to  themselves 
by  facial  gestuies  and  excessive  handling. 

Could  it  be  that  such  mothers  just  don't  know  how  to  handle  babies? 

"Most  of  these  motheis  come  from  the  lower  sociocconomic  levels," 
Dr.  Greenberg  answers,  "so  there  may  be  an  educational  factor  involved. 
But  I  don't  know  that  to  be  a  good  mother  requires  formal  education;  I 
think  it  icquires  an  adequate  personality."  Most  of  the  45  mothers  in  the 
group  studied  so  far  have  been  found  to  have  major  emotional 
disturbances.  They  are  usually  irritable,  distant,  neglectful,  depressed,  and 
markedly  anxious,  the  investigator  finds.  They  go  from  one  crisis  to 
another. 


Troubled  Children 

When  the  mother  of  a  normal  infant  leaves  him  alone  in  the  labora- 
tory's nursery  and  then  a  stranger  comes  in,  the  investigator's  films  show, 
the  child  is  apt  to  cry  or  sob  for  a  while.  Not  so  an  infant  with  develop- 
mental problems.  Such  a  child  either  continues  to  do  whatever  he  has 
been  doing,  without  any  sign  of  acknowledging  the  separation;  or  he 
retreats,  or  sits  or  lies  motionless,  or  begins  body-rocking;  or  he  engages  in 
extreme  crying  and  cannot  be  comforted  by  being  held,  played  with,  or 
even  fed.  The  crying  in  the  last  situation  is  not  weeping,  Dr.  Greenberg 
observes,  but  straightforward  shrieking. 

Unlike  most  children  who  have  been  raised  at  home,  however,  these 
babies  with  developmental  problems  apparently  reach  the  age  of  8  or  9 
months  without  having  established  object  specificity,  meaning  the  ability 
to  show  a  preference  for  one  or  more  of  the  persons  around  them.  When 
one  of  these  infants  has  to  be  separated  from  his  mother  in  order  to  be 
hospitalized,  he  generally  accepts  the  ward  and  cheers  tip  more  quickly 
than  other  babies.  In  Dr.  Greenberg's  words,  these  infants  have  a 
gregariousness  that  lacks  definition  and  singularity,  with  the  result  that 

205 


everybody  becomes  an  acquaintance  but  no  one  a  solid  friend.  Whether  o, 
not  such  a  state,  with  its  portent  of  trouble  later  on,  will  continue  remaitu 
to  be  seen. 

The  most  marked  symptom  in  several  of  the  children  is  pica,  a  craving 
for  unnatural  foods.  This  has  become  a  public  health  problem  of  some 
size,  according  to  Dr.  Greenbeig,  because  the  afflicted  babies  often  eat 
paint-covered  plaster  and  then  develop  lead  poisoning,  which  affects  the 
central  nervous  system  and  may  result  in  convulsions  and  death.  (In  an 
attempt  to  find  the  lead-eateis  early  enough,  teams  of  doctors  and  nurses 
are  going  into  homes  in  some  parts  of  Chicago  and  collecting  urine 
samples  for  analysis.) 

Pica  children  apparently  feel  deprived  and  hungry,  but  just  what 
happens  in  the  mothei -infant  relationship  to  give  rise  to  their  condition, 
says  Dr.  Greenberg,  is  not  yet  clear.  Interestingly,  the  films  taken  in  the 
laboratory  nursery  do  indicate  that  the  mothers  of  pica  babies— in  contrast 
to  most  of  the  other  mothers—act  normally  in  the  feeding  situation. 

The  investigators  have  encountered  even  worse  problems.  One  of  the 
first  children  referred  to  Dr.  Greenberg,  for  example,  had  been  admitted 
to  the  Univeisity  of  Illinois  Hospital  at  the  age  of  7  months  with  a  com- 
mon trouble,  failure  to  thrive.  "She  was  quiet  and  somewhat  detached  and 
withdrawn,"  Dr.  Greenbeig  reports,  "and  frequently  nodded  her  head  in 
the  typical  movements  of  spasmus  nutans  (head-rolling).  She  turned  away 
from  the  observers,  although  she  was  attentive  to  auditory  and  visual 
stimuli.  She  showed  no  reactions  of  pleasure,  and  we  were  unable  to  make 
her  smile.  She  sucked  eagerly  when  she  was  held  and  fed,  but  was 
interested  only  in  the  bottle  and  paid  no  attention  to  the  feeder's  face.1" 
Moving  pictures  made  in  the  laboratory  nursery  indicated  that  Betty 
was  getting  a  fantastic  type  of  attention.  On  one  occasion,  for  example, 
the  mother  reached  into  the  crib,  pulled  Betty's  fingers  from  her  mouth 
and  began  kneading  the  baby's  face  and  head  almost  as  if  they  were  dough 
01-  clay.  Betty  began  fussing.  The  mother  explained  that  she  was  ac- 
customed to  rubbing  the  baby's  face  and  body  for  hoius  at  a  time  "to 
build  her  up." 

Betty  soon  gained  weight  in  the  hospital  and  improved  in  other  ways, 
She  became  curious  and  responsive,  smiled,  reached  for  toys  and  people. 
Her  head-nodding  stopped  within  a  week-and-a-half, 

After  Betty  was  discharged,  the  mother  brought  her  back  several  times 
so  that  the  team  could  continue  to  study  the  mother-infant  relationship 
and  try  to  help  the  mother  with  her  own  emotional  problems.  When  she 
complained  of  brutality  to  Betty  by  the  people  they  were  living  with,  the 
doctors  and  their  staff  even  helped  the  family  move  into  a  separate  apart- 
ment, A  month  later,  however,  Betty  was  returned  to  the  hospital  in  a 
coma  and  died  of  a  fractured  skull. 

Since  then,  several  other  battered  babies,  among  those  admitted  to  the 
hospital,  have  been  added  to  Dr.  Greenberg's  cases.  Brutality  to  infants  isj 


Greenbeig,  N.  H.  Origins  of  head-rolling  (spasmus  nutans)  during  early  infancy. 
Psychosom.  Med.,  1964,  26,  2. 


more  common  than  generally  realized,  and  its  impact  on  mental  health  in 
later  years  may  well  be  shown— through  studies  like  Dr.  Greenberg's-to  be 
grievous.  The  investigator  calls  attention  to  suggestions  that  schizo- 
phrenics have  experienced  not  only  harshness  during  infancy  but  also 
physical  cruelty. 


Treatment 

If  we  know  how  emotional  disorders  originate,  perhaps  we  can  step  in 
to  prevent  them  or  at  least  to  shorten  the  treatment  and  make  its  effects 
more  enduring.  And  the  logical  approach  in  cases  like  those  understudy, 
where  physical  symptoms  point  to  psychological  problems,  Dr.  Greenberg 
believes,  is  to  try  to  restore  the  babies'  normal  developmental  channels  by 
removing  the  pathogenic  factors  in  the  infant-mother  relationship. 

Many  of  the  mothers  in  this  particular  study,  though,  show  no 
conscious  concern  for  their  babies  and  therefore  resist  therapy  for  them- 
selves. About  the  only  thing  psychotherapy  has  accomplished  so  far,  Dr. 
Greenberg  finds,  is  to  help  some  of  the  motheis  get  through  periods  of 
extreme  anxiety.  If  the  study  clearly  demonstrates  that  certain  maternal 
responses  are  harmful,  lie  hopes  eventually  to  let  mothers  see  in  moving 
pictures  how  they  are  reacting  to  their  babies'  needs.  Then  perhaps  the 
point  can  be  made  that  certain  aspects  of  their  behavior  are  not  ideal. 

When  the  infants  arc  removed  from  what  the  investigator  terms  their 
psychotoxic  environment,  many  improve  quickly.  The  most  dramatic 
results  occur  in  babies  only  a  few  months  old  who  have  feeding  disorders. 
Within  a  week  after  they  have  been  hospitalized,  these  babies  often  stop 
regurgitating,  begin  eating  normally,  and  gain  in  weight.  Further,  they 
show  less  withdrawal  and  apathy  and  more  curiosity  and  social  responsive- 
ness. 

Among  the  unanswered  questions:  How  long  will  the  improvement  last? 
Why  do  some  of  the  babies,  particularly  those  with  motility  disorders, 
show  no  substantial  change  in  the  new  environment? 

Dr.  Greenberg  points  to  another  aspect  of  the  situation:  some  mothers 
subconsciously  do  not  want  their  children  to  become  completely  well— 
they  need  sick  babies. 


Institutional  vs.  Family  Babies 

In  preliminary  work  several  years  ago,  Dr.  Greenberg  found  decided 
differences  between  institutional  babies  from  the  foundling  home,  and 
family  babies  being  reared  by  their  own  parents.  One  difference  lay  in  the 
babies'  responses  to  pacification  attempts  when  they  were  in  distress.  The 
institutional  babies  as  a  group  were  easier  to  pacify  than  the  others,  and  at 
one  month  of  age  could  be  pacified  equally  well  by  any  of  three  tech- 
niques-feeding them,  giving  them  a  nipple  pacifier,  or  holding  them  in  a 
sitting  position  in  their  cribs.  The  family  babies,  on  the  other  hand,  could 

207 


be  smoothed  much  more  easily  by  either  of  the  first  two  methods  tlun 
the  third.  As  the  babies  were  pacified,  their  heatt  rates  dropped,  hut  th1 
heait  rates  dropped  considerably  lower  among  the  institutional  babies. 

These  findings  indicate,  the  investigator  suggests,  that  family  babk 
may  organize  their  response  patteins  earlier-that  is,  show  a  prefcrciitt 
among  pacification  attempts  earlier.  In  addition  the  findings  may  intfieali 
that  the  babies  raised  in  the  nuisery  (I)  are  more  sensitive  than  the  other; 
to  pacification  effoits,  and  (2)  have  less  effective  feedback  mechanisms 
for  controlling  the  heart  rate.  The  increased  sensitivity  could  be  explained 
by  the  conditions  of  sensoiy  lestriction  under  which  infants  arc  being 
laised.  In  this  study  the  attention  given  a  nursery  baby— for  feeding, 
batliing,  and  ail  other  purposes-was  always  less  than  2  hours,  and  some- 
times as  little  as  an  hour  and  a  quarter,  in  a  24-hour  day.  The  aides  rarely 
sang  or  spoke  to  the  infants,  and  the  fcedings-for  which  nipples  with 
large  holes  were  used— were  often  very  rapid. 

Another  difference  was  in  weight.  Even  though  the  institutional 
were  on  a  more  than  adequate  diet  and  were  getting  excellent 
care,  by  the  end  of  the  first  2  months  they  had  gained  only  half  IKS  much 
as  the  other  babies.  Dr.  Greenberg  believes  that  this  difference,  too,  is 
attributable  to  the  restricted  sensory  environment  of  the  nursery  infants 
At  tins  point  there  is  no  evidence  to  suggest  that  the  observed  dif- 
ferences are  permanent  or  that  they  have  implications  for  personality 
development.  In  the  matter  of  weight,  in  fact,  there  is  evidence-given  (he 
investigator  by  a  number  of  adoptive  mothers-that  institutional  babies 
quickly  catch  up  after  they  have  been  adopted.  The  findings  do  indicate 
to  Dr.  Greenberg  that  the  nature  and  the  extent  of  the  opportunities  for 
communication  between  the  infant  and  its  surroundings  can  affect 
physiological  function  and  behavioral  activity  in  infancy. 

Tliis  earlier  work  is  being  repeated  and  extended  with  the  groups  of 
infants  now  under  study. 

For  the  Future 

In  order  to  study  the  frequency  of  developmental  problems,  the  ic- 
search  group  is  sending  pediatric  checklists  to  each  mothei  who  dclivcis  nl 
the  University  of  Illinois  Hospital.  The  lists  enable  her  to  describe  quickly 
virtually  all  aspects  of  her  baby's  behavior.  Lists  go  out  at  3,  8,  and  IS 
months,  and  many  of  the  mothers  check  and  return  them.  If  the  baby's 
behavior  appears  to  be  abnormal,  the  mother  is  asked  to  bring  him  in  for  u 
checkup,  and  she  may  then  be  invited  to  participate  in  the  study  being 
conducted  by  Dr.  Greenberg. 

In  still  another  effort  to  get  precise  information  about  the  conditions 
leading  to  emotional  disturbance,  Dr.  Greenberg's  team  is  beginning  to 
survey  2,000  babies  a  year  born  to  families  using  the  well-baby  clinics  of 
the  Infant  Welfare  Society.  Dr.  Greenberg  explains  that  the  society 
sponsors  one  of  the  few  chad  guidance  centers  in  Chicago  that  treat 
emotionally  disturbed  children  of  preschool  age.  Eventually  a  few  of  the 
babies  being  surveyed  will  develop  emotional  disorders  and  be  referred  to 

208 


this  center  for  treatment.  Then  the  research  group  will  be  able  to  look  for 
a  relationship  between  the  circumstances  of  infancy  and  the  types  of 
emotional  disturbance  that  develop— or,  as  the  investigator  sums  it  up, 
who  gets  what. 

Most  of  the  babies  now  under  study  because  they  have  developmental 
disorders  come  from  families  at  the  lower  socioeconomic  levels.  Would 
babies  fiom  other  types  of  families  show  the  same  troubles?  Dr. 
Greenberg  doesn't  know,  but  talks  with  other  psychiatrists  and  with 
pediatricians  have  given  him  the  impiession  that  babies  from  middle  and 
upper  middle-class  families  have  a  much  lower  incidence  of  feeding 
pioblems  and  of  hypermotility.  Developmental  disorders  in  these  babies 
seem  to  be  related  somehow  to  sleep  disturbances.  Sometime  in  the  future 
the  investigatoi  hopes  to  check  this  point  by  studying  infants  from 
Chicago's  North  Shore,  where  most  of  the  residents  aie  professional 
people. 


Adopted  Children 

Studies  in  child  guidance  clinics  and  mental  hospitals  indicate  that 
adopted  children  have  a  higher  incidence  of  a  variety  of  psychiatric  prob- 
lems, but  these  studies  generally  have  not  taken  into  account  either  the 
age  of  adoption  or  the  time  that  may  have  been  spent  in  an  institution 
before  adoption.  Dr.  Greenberg  believes  both  factors  to  be  critical.  The 
babies  in  his  institutional  group,  to  be  followed  through  the  years,  will  all 
have  been  adopted  at  the  same  age  of  2  months.  Eventually  he  hopes  to 
compare  their  emotional  well-being  not  only  with  that  of  the  normative 
group,  but  also  with  that  of  some  of  the  children  from  the  foundling 
home  who  were  adopted  just  a  few  days  after  birth.  In  the  case  of  the 
2-month  gioup,  he  also  hopes  to  throw  light  on  a  largely  unexplored  area, 
the  nature  of  the  parent-child  relationships  in  adoptive  families. 


Rescaich  Grant:   MH  5527 

Careei  Program  Development  Award:  K3-MH  13,984 

Date  of  Intei  view:  June  5,  1964 

References: 

Greenberg,  N.  H.  Studies  in  psychosomatic  differentiation  during  infancy.  Arch,  gen, 

Psychiflt.t  1962,7,389. 
Greenberg,  N.  H.  Origins  of  head-rolling  (spasmus  nutans)  during  early  infancy. 

Psychos-am.  Med,,  1964,26,2. 
Greenberg,  N.  H.  Developmental  effects  of  stimulation  during  early  infancy.  Ann  N.  Y. 

Acad,  Sci,,  in  press,  1965. 
Greenberg,  N.  H.,  Cekan,  P.,  &  Loesch,  J,  G.  Some  cardiac  rate  and  behavioral 

characteristics  of  sucking  in  the  neonate.  Presented  at  annual  meetings  Amcr. 

Psychosomat.Soc,,  Atlantic  City,  1963. 
Greenberg,  N.  H.,  &  Loesch,  J.  G.  Celiac  disease.  Draft,  1963. 

209 


Greenberg,  N,  H.,  &.  Loesch,  J.  G.  A  comparative  study  of  some  behavior; 

physiological  activities  m  nursery  and  family-ieared  infants  during  the  f 

months  of  infancy.  Presented  at  annual  meeting  Ainer,  Orthopsychiat. 

Chicago,  1960. 
Greenberg,  N.  H.,  Loesch,  J.  G.,  &  Lipgar,  R.  Preliminary  observations  on  symi 

foiination  during  infancy.  Dratt,  1 964. 


Investigator; 

Margaret  S.Mahler,  M.D. 

Masters  Children's  Center 

New  York,N.Y. 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Several  times  a  week,  a  number  of  young  mothers  in  the  Greenwich 
Village  section  of  New  York  City  spend  a  morning  or  an  afternoon  in  an 
old  brownstone  house  on  Horatio  Street.  Their  children  play  or  sleep.  The 
mothers  care  for  them  as  usual,  talk  to  one  another,  or  read.  All  the  while 
they  are  being  closely  observed  by  a  research  team,  and  the  mothers  know 
it.  The  young  women  who  serve  as  playroom  teachers  are  actually  trained 
observers,  and  one  of  them  slips  out  every  half  hour  to  dictate  a  report  on 
the  activities  and  attitudes  of  one  of  the  mothers  and  her  child.  Other 
observers,  out  of  sight,  make  notes  from  booths.  Even  the  room  where  the 
diapers  are  changed  has  an  observation  window. 

Never  before,  it  is  believed,  have  normal  mothers  and  noimal  children 
been  studied  so  intensively  over  the  period  in  which  the  investigators  arc 
interested-frorn  the  age  of  about  5  months  to  the  end  of  the  third  year. 
The  findings  are  expected  to  increase  our  undeistanding  of  why  some 
children  become  schizophrenic  or  show  other  signs  of  emotional  dis- 
turbance and  to  improve  our  ability  to  prevent  such  developments. 

The  brownstone  house  is  the  home  of  the  Masters  Children's  Center,  an 
organization  for  the  study  and  treatment  of  disturbed  children  and  the 
study  of  normal  children.  Dr.  Margaret  S.  Mahler,  the  internationally 
known  children's  psychoanalyst  who  directs  the  center's  research  program 
(and  is  clinical  professor  of  psychiatry  at  Albert  Einstein  College  of 
Medicine),  believes  that  the  roots  of  much  childhood  mental  illness  are  to 
be  found  in  the  relations  between  mother  and  child  during  the  period, 
beginning  when  the  child  is  about  5  months  old,  in  which  their  initial 
oneness  begins  to  slowly  come  apart.  Dr.  Mahler  calls  this  theseparation- 
individuation  phase  of  child  development,  meaning  the  phase  in  which  the 
child  gradually  separates  from  his  extremely  close,  symbiotic  union  with 
his  mother  and  comes  to  recognize  himself  as  an  individual.  In  the 
investigator's  words  this  is  the  period  in  which  the  child  hatches  from  the 
symbiotic  membrane  and  becomes  as  individuated  toddler.  If  something 
goes  wrong  with  the  hatching  process,  Dr.  Mahler  theorizes,  emotional 
problems  develop;  if  something  goes  very  wrong,  the  problems  may  be 
severe  and  in  extreme  cases  may  lead  to  the  type  of  schizophrenia  she  has 
described  as  symbiotic  child  psychosis. 

211 


H  was  through  hei  work  with  psychotic  children  that  Dr.  Ma 
became  aware  of  the  impoitance  of  this  period  of  development.  In  in 
cases  of  symbiotic  child  psychosis,  the  symptoms  could  be  explains 
seemed  to  her,  only  by  supposing  that  the  child  for  some  reason  had 
successfully  come  through  the  hatching  process  and  was  trying  frantic; 
to  regain  union  with  his  mother. 

But  just  what  had  gone  wrong?  And  when?  And  might  it  have  b( 
prevented? 

She  found  she  could  offer  only  theoretical  answers,  because  this  peri 
of  child  development  had  never  been  studied   intensively   enough 
provide  a  detailed  description  of  what  happens  normally.  To  help  supp 
the  needed  infoimation,  some  of  the  staff  of  the  Masters  Children's  Cent 
made  a  pilot  study  of  mothers  and  children  and  reported  that  in  tl 
normal  course  of  events  the  separation-individuation  phase  apparent! 
comprises  four  major  parts.  In  each  of  these  the  child  is  developing  bol 
physically  and  psychologically;  in  each  he  is  called  upon  to  accompli? 
certain  tasks. 

The  current  study  was  undertaken  in  1963  to  verify  these  findings  am 
to  set  forth  in  detail  the  mother-child  relationship  and  the  child's  boliavio 
patterns  that  ate  characteristic  of  each  subphase.  Heading  the  staff  assist 
ing  Dr.  Mahler  and  Dr.  John  McDevitt,  a  psychoanalyst,  and  Dr.  Kitty 
LaPerriere,  a  psychologist.  The  information  is  collected  by  observing 
mother  and  child  at  the  Center,  interviewing  the  mother  every  week, 
making  a  long,  informal  visit  to  the  home  every  2  months,  and  occasional- 
ly observing  father  and  child.  The  babies  are  tested  periodically,  and 
moving  pictures  of  mother-child  interaction  are  made  both  at  the  Ceuta 
and  in  the  home. 

The  families  in  the  study,  all  of  whom  live  within  walking  distance  of 
the  Center,  are  described  as  middle  class  and  Protestant;  most  of  the 
fathers  and  many  of  the  mothers  are  college  graduates.  The  mo  the  is  m 
happy  to  participate  in  the  research,  Dr.  Mahler  reports,  because  the 
Center  provides  excellently  equipped  playrooms  for  their  youngsters  and 
emotional  support  to  themselves,  and  because  the  mothers  like  to  fed  tlial 
they  and  their  children  are  serving  the  cause  of  mental  health.  The  Center 
is  open  to  them  3  days  a  week;  most  of  them  come  in  for  two  or  three 
mornings  or  afternoons.  By  the  time  the  project  ends,  in  1 968,  more  than 
40  children  and  their  mothers,  half  of  them  in  the  course  of  the  less- 
detailed  pilot  study,  will  have  been  observed  during  the  separation- 
individuation  process. 

One  of  the  objectives  of  the  research  is  to  point  to  the  danger  signals  in 
each  phase  of  that  process~the  indications  that  child  and  mother  are  not 
successfully  coping  with  the  problems  natural  to  a  given  stage  of  develop- 
ment and  that  they  are,  consequently,  storing  up  problems  for  later  stages. 
It  is  hoped  that  the  children  now  under  study  can  be  compared  with  one 
another  at  various  periods,  into  adulthood.  Some  of  the  children  appear  to  ' 
be  proceeding  through  the  separation-individuation  phase  more  successful- 
ly than  others,  and  a  variety  of  at  least  temporary  problems  are  being 
encountered.  A  long-term  follow-up,  the  investigators  explain,  would  be 


the  best  possible  way  of  establishing  a  link  between  certain  events  or 
patterns  in  this  phase  and  the  quality  of  mental  health  in  later  life. 

As  discussed  later,  the  investigators  think  they  have  already  spotted  a 
few  of  the  danger  signals.  The  chief  general  conclusion  emerging  from  the 
work  so  far  is  that  the  child's  psychological  development  depends  in  large 
measure  upon  the  "emotional  availability"  of  the  mother.  In  all  phases  of 
the  separation-individuation  process,  he  needs  to  be  sure  of  her  interest 
and  love. 

The  Roots  of  Childhood  Schizophrenia 

Discussing  her  earlier  work  with  psychotic  children  and  their  mothers, 
Dr.  Mahler  recalls  an  experiment  by  another  investigator,  in  which  chick 
embiyos  and  newly  hatched  chicks  were  pricked  with  a  pin.  The  chicks 
that  had  hatched  showed  only  infinitesimal  damage  but  those  still  in  the 
shell  developed  majoi  anatomical  defects. 

"The  lesson  is  applicable  to  the  child's  psychological  development,"  Dr. 
Mahler  observes.  "Theie  is  a  natural  timetable  foi  the  maluiation  and 
development  of  the  controlling,  steering,  integrating  part  of  the  peison- 
ality,  the  ego.  The  earlier  in  life  that  this  timetable  is  interrupted,  the 
more  detrimental  to  the  total  personality."  From  her  previous  research, 
the  investigator  believes  that  in  childhood  schizophrenia  the  timetable  has 
been  intenupted  very  early,  either  before  or  not  long  aftei  the  beginning 
of  the  separation-individuation  process. 

Because  of  disturbed  or  insensitive  motheis,  some  of  the  children  who 
later  developed  schizophrenia  had  suffered  numerous  and  severe  frustra- 
tions and  emotional  trauma  during  the  first  few  months  of  life.  At  one 
extreme  there  was  the  depressed  mother  who  could  show  no  sign  of 
affection  for  her  daughter.  At  the  other  extreme  there  was  the  smothering 
and  overwhelmingly  affectionate  mother  who  constantly  overstimulated 
her  son  and  showed  no  comprehension  of  his  need  to  experience  life  at  his 
own  pace. 

But  Dr.  Mahler  found  just  as  many  cases  of  childhood  psychosis  in 
which  the  mother  had  been  of  at  least  average  competence  and  devotion. 
In  some  of  these  cases  the  child  had  shown  such  an  extreme  vul- 
nerability—had behaved  so  abnormally  almost  fiom  birth— that  not  even 
the  most  favorable  environmental  situation,  the  investigator  is  convinced, 
could  have  prevented  psychosis.  In  such  cases,  she  says  the  vulnerability  is 
"seemingly  intrinsic,"  or  congenital. 

Whether  tl;e  disposition  to  childhood  schizophrenia  is  innate  or 
acquired,  Dr.  Mahler  believes  that  the  core  of  the  problem  is  the  same;  an 
inability  on  the  part  of  the  child  to  use  the  mother  as  a  "beacon  of 
orientation."  She  points  out  that  an  infant's  mental  appaiatus  is  too 
undeveloped  to  organize  and  act  upon  the  stimuli  he  receives  from  within 
and  without;  hence  his  survival  depends  upon  his  mother.  His  psycho- 
logical development  also  depends  upon  her.  Even  after  he  has  passed  the 
completely  helpless  stage,  he  looks  to  her  foi  guidance  and  strength  in  a 
world  that  can  be  terrifyingly— as  well  as  enticingly— strange. 

213 


in  me  cype  ot  childhood  schizophienia  described  asattfism,  Dr.  M; 
theorizes  that  the  baby's  personality  development  has  failed  to  n 
beyond  (or  else  has  regressed  to)  the  ''normal  autistic  phase"  of  dcve 
merit.  During  this  phase,  lasting  from  birth  until  sometime  in  the  sec 
month,  the  infant  seems  completely  unaware  of  any  distinction  beUv 
himself  and  the  world  around  him.  The  most  conspicuous  symptom  of 
autistic  child,  says  Dr.  Mahler,  is  his  apparently  complete  failure 
perceive  his  mother  as  a  living  being  who  represents  the  outside  world  i 
serves  as  a  beacon  and  guide.  Mothers  say:  "I  never  could  reach 

baby "  "He  never  greeted  me  when  I  entered,  he  never  cried  or  ci 

noticed  when  I  left  the  room. .  .  ."  "She  never  made  any  peisonal  tipp 
for  help  at  any  time."  Such  a  child  seeks  refuge  in  an  "autistic  shell,"  t 
investigator  believes,  because  he  appaiently  has  experienced  reiiJity-a 
even  his"  mother-as  an  intolerable  source  of  initation;  he  has  defend 
himself  by  warding  off  reality  and  withdrawing  into  an  iitler 
constricted, deammated  world. 

In  symbiotic  child  psychosis,  according  to  Dr.  Mahler's  theory,  fl 
trouble  with  the  baby*s  personality  development  arises  at  a  later  stage  an 
results  from  an  imbalance  between  the  rates  at  which  the  child  is  mntitrin 
physically  and  emotionally. 

As  the  investigator  explains  it,  a  maturational  spurt  puts  the  nonnt 
toddler,  in  his  second  year,  in  the  position  of  relatively  advanced  physica 
independence.  But  in  some  cases  his  emotional  development  seems  lolai 
far  behind-  Physically,  in  such  a  case,  he  is  able  to  move  away  from  hi' 
mother;  emotionally  he  is  not.  So  he  is  bewildered  and  panicky.  Miuoi 
frustrations  common  to  the  early  part  of  the  separation-indivMuation 
phase   bring  extreme  reactions.   He  may  give  up  walking  for  montlis 
because  of  a  fall.  Typically,  the  break  with  reality  is  triggered  by  some 
event-such  as  enrollment  in  nursery  school,  the  birth  of  a  sibling,  hos- 
pitalization-that  makes  him  fear  loss  of  or  separation  from  his  mother 
and  throws  him  into  a  panic  he  cannot  handle.  In  his  hallucinations  he 
appears  to  be  trying  to  restore  the  delusion  of  an  earlier  period  that  lie 
and  his  mother  are  cue.  Since  constant  panic  is  unbearable,  the  child 
retreats  into  "secondary  autism"  and  cuts  himself  off  from  the  world. 
Though  his  behavior  is  then  like  that  seen  in  autism,  says  Dr.  Mahler, 
successful   treatment  depends  upon  realizing  that  his  personality  hns 
developed  further,  to  a  stage  in  the  process  by  which  a  child  separates 
from  his  mother.  The  goal  of  therapy,  she  believes,  is  to  enable  the  mother 
and  the  child  to  reestablish  the  symbiotic  tie,  and  then  to  help  them  move 
forward  through  the  separation-individuation  phase. 
During  the  earlier  research,  Dr.  Mahler  and  her  associates  were  able  to 
lieve  this  goal  in  about  half  of  the  dozen  child-and-mother  cases  treated 
tir  of  the  others  withdrew  from  treatment  against  professional  advice). 
erapist  saw  mother  and  child  together -several  times  a  week  and  for 
as  I  W~  at  a  time.  The  therapist's  first  task,  the  investigator 
e  her  presence  felt -to  allow  the  child  to  experience  it 
e  without  having  to  acknowledge  her  existence  as  a 
er  presence  became  a  soothing  phenomenon,  and  the 


child  felt  more  comfortable  with  it  than  without  it.  Then  the  child  came 
to  use  the  therapist  as  an  extension  of  himself— a  tool  to  reach  something, 
a  soft  platform  to  learn  against.  Eventually  he  allowed  her  to  meet  his 
needs  more  actively— to  feed  him,  to  play  with  him. 

"The  theory  is  that  the  therapist  represents  a  mothering  principle,  not  a 
distinct  human  object,"  Dr  Mahler  says.  "The  situation  seems  to  be 
comparable  to  that  stage  in  development  when  the  baby  dimly  recognizes 
that  ministrations  iclieving  distress  come  from  outside  himself.  It  is  likely 
that  the  therapist's  comforting  piesence  helps  to  amelioiate  some  of  the 
child's  intense  aggression  and  destructiveness  and,  therefore,  reduces  the 
level  of  anxiety."  Gradually,  and  in  the  face  of  considerable  resentment  at 
having  to  relate  to  her  preschooler  as  to  an  infant,  the  mother  was  led  into 
the  kind  of  relationship  with  the  child  that  had  been  established  by  the 
therapist. 

The  present  reseaich,  with  normal  children  and  their  mothers,  was 
undertaken  both  to  check  Dr.  Mahler's  ideas  about  the  critical  importance 
of  the  separation-individuation  phase  and  to  help  establish— as  an  aid  to 
workers  in  the  field  of  child  mental  health— the  normal  course  of  events 
during  that  period. 


Stages  of  Development 


From  the  work  to  date,  Dr.  Mahler  offers  the  following  picture  of  a 
child's  development  following  the  "normal  autistic  phase"  of  the  first  few 
weeks  of  life. 

Beginning  about  the  second  month  and  continuing  into  the  fifth,  the 
infant  is  in  the  symbiotic  phase.  The  boundaries  between  himself  and  his 
mother  still  tend  to  merge  but  he  seems  to  be  dimly  aware-particularly 
when  he  is  being  fed,  changed,  or  actively  cared  for  in  other  ways-that  his 
needs  are  being  met  by  something  outside  of  himself, 

By  the  fifth  or  sixth  month,  the  infant  seems  to  recognize  that  the 
object  through  which  his  gratifications  are  provided  and  his  discomfort 
relieved  is  his  mother.  When  her  face  is  near,  lie  tries  to  touch  and 
investigate  it;  he  watches  her  play  peek-a-boo  and  then  plays  it  himself. 
This  is  the  beginning  of  the  separation-individuation  phase,  the  subject  of 
the  current  research. 

The  first  4  to  6  months  of  this  stage  are  found  to  comprise  a  fairly 
distinct  subphase  which  Dr.  Mahler  labels  differentiation.  It  is  marked  not 
only  by  explorations  of  the  mother's  face  and  mouth  and  hair  but  also  by 
a  turning  to  the  outside  world  for  pleasure  and  stimulation.  The  infant 
looks  beyond  neaiby  objects.  He  takes  a  more  active  interest  in  toys.  He 
finds  pleasure  in  using  his  whole  body.  He  begins  to  creep,  climb,  and 
stand  up.  He  makes  progress  in  coordinating  the  use  of  eyes,  hands,  and 
mouth.  During  this  period  the  children  at  the  Center  were  seen  to  be 
active  for  a  longer  time  and  to  be  more  vivacious  when  their  mothers  were 
close.  They  showed  a  distinct  preference  for  playing  at  their  mothers'  feet. 

215 


Beginning  as  early  as  the  10th  month  and  as  late  as  the  12th  comes  ( 
subphase  desciibed  as  practicing  during  which  the  child  takes  delight 
trying  out  his  new  skills,  particularly  his  ability  to  get  around  by  hiinsc 
Eager  to  explore  his  environment,  he  ciawls  or  walks  farther  and  nut! 
from  his  mother's  feet  and  often  becomes  so  absorbed  in  his  own  activiti 
that  for  long  periods  of  time  he  seems  to  be  oblivious  to  his  mollio 
presence. 

The  infant's  dominant  mood  during  this  practicing  period,  the  inves 
gator  repoits,  is  elation.  As  another  authority  on  children  has  expressed 
the  toddler  is  now  beginning  his  "love  affair  with  the  woild."  There 
such  wonderment  to  be  explored  that  he  often  disiegards  bumps,  fall 
and  other  frustrations.  Sometimes  at  the  Center  he  slips  out  of  the  infii 
room,  where  his  mother  is  sitting,  and  makes  his  way  into  the  adjacei 
toddler  room  which  is  equipped  with  toys  and  apparatus  for  the  olcf 
children.  But  in  the  midst  of  his  investigations  he  stops  as  if  sudden 
aware  that  something  is  wrong  and  goes  back  rapidly  to  his  mother,  onl 
to  venture  forth  again  a  little  later. 

A  child  during  this  phase  seems  to  need  peiiodic  physical  contact  wit 
his  mother.  "We  see  babies  crawling  to  the  mother,  righting  themselves  o 
her  leg  or  touching  hei  in  othei  ways,"  says  Dr.  Mahler,  "or  just  stnnclfn 
and  leaning  against  her  for  'emotional  lefueling.'  Even  a  wilting,  fatigue 
infant  perks  up  in  a  very  short  time  upon  such  contact." 


The  Period  of  "Rapprochement" 

At  18  months,  or  a  little  eailier  01  later,  the  child  gradually  passes  ind 
a  stage  when  he  is  more  subdued  and  even  a  little  troubled.  He  has  bcei 
acting  during  the  practicing  period  as  though  he  were  omnipotent  and  tin 
world  were  his  for  the  taking.  Now,  Dr.  Mahler  reports,  it  dawns  on  bin 
that  he  has  to  cope  with  the  world  as  an  individual  who  is  very  small 
relatively  helpless,  separate,  and  lonely.  Watching  him  in  the  playroom 
the  investigators  notice  that  he  is  no  longer  either  relatively  unaware  of  life 
mother  or  unmindful  of  frustrations.  And  they  interpret  some  of  hi1 
expiessions  as  indicating  surprise  at  finding  himself  a  separate  being,  Foi 
example,  says  Dr.  Mahler,  when  he  hurts  himelf  he  looks  perplexed 
because  his  mother  is  not-instantly  and  automatically-with  him.  As  UK 
toddler  realizes  his  physical  ability  to  move  away  from  his  mother,  he 
generally  seems  to  have  a  great  need  for  her  to  share  every  new  skill  und 
experience.  The  observers'  notes  and  the  moving  pictures  show  that 
children  during  this  third  stage  of  the  separation-individuation  process  are 
continually  concerned  with  their  mothers1  whereabouts. 

Those  watching  a  youngster  week  after  week  report  than  in  this  phase 
of  development  the  toddler's  pleasure  in  functioning  on  his  own  is 
proportionate  to  his  success  in  eliciting  his  mother's  interest  and  participa- 
tion in  his  activities.  When  the  mother  leaves  the  room  the  child- 
sometimes  only  by  a  fleeting  expression  of  sadness  or  anxiety,  sometimes 
by  a  tantrum-displays  unhappiness.  Sometimes  he  goes  and  stands  by  or 

216 


climbs  onto  her  chair,  taking  it  over  for  a  while  as  a  substitute  for  hei.  He 
does  not  easily  accpet  physical  contact  with  people  who  offer  themselves 
as  substitutes.  Because  of  this  behavior,  Dr.  Mahler  calls  this  third  stage 
the  period  of  rapprochement  For  most  of  the  youngsters  studied,  it  is  a 
time  when  indwiduation  proceeds  rapidly,  but  separation  is  resisted 
strongly.  It  is  a  period  that  is  conducive  to  misunderstandings  between 
mother  and  child.  Some  mothers  cannot  accept  the  child's  demandingness, 
cannot  undei  stand  why  a  toddlei  who  is  obviously  more  capable  and 
independent  than  he  was  a  few  months  ago  during  the  practicing  period, 
must  now  insist  upon  her  sharing  every  aspect  of  his  life. 

As  the  investigate!  explains  it,  the  child  needs  his  mother's  active 
emotional  support  at  this  time  in  older  to  prevent  seiious  injury  to  his 
self-esteem.  He  has  had  inklings  that  he  is  not  really  the  omnipotent  being 
he  had  fancied  himself.  If  he  can  count  on  backing  from  his  mother,  he 
comes  giadually  to  accept  this  fact  and  to  pour  his  energy  into  normal 
psychological  development.  "If  the  mother  is  quietly  available,"  Dr. 
Mahler  says,  "if  she  shares  the  toddling  adventurer's  exploits,  playfully 
reciprocates  and  thus  helps  his  attempts  at  individuation,  the  relationship 
between  mother  and  toddler  progresses  to  the  point  where  verbal  com- 
munication takes  over.  Emotional  paiticipation  that  the  toddler  can  count 
upon  seems  to  facilitate  the  rich  unfolding  of  his  thought  pioccsses. 

"The  less  emotionally  available  the  mother  has  become,"  The  investi- 
gator repoits,  "the  more  insistently  and  even  desperately  the  toddler 
attempts  to  woo  her."  He  won't  accept  comfoiting  from  anyone  else;  he 
can't  seem  to  lose  himself  in  play.  If  emotional  supplies  are  not  forth- 
coming, he  seeks  substitutes  in  eating  and  sucking,  He  also  turns  to  such 
aggressive  behavior  as  throwing  things  and  hitting  people.  All  this  may 
drain  so  much  of  the  energy  available  for  development,  Di.  Mahler 
theorizes,  as  to  hamper  psychological  growth. 

One  sign  of  an  unusual  degree  of  conflict  in  the  youngster,  the  investi- 
gator believes,  is  a  more  than  average  amount  of  "shadowing"-that  is, 
keeping  the  mother  in  his  sight  even  in  the  midst  of  play,  and  at  other 
times  following  her  around.  Another  sign  is  an  exaggerated  use  of  the 
game  of  daiting  away  in  order  to  provoke  the  mother  into  pursuing  him 
and  scooping  him  up.  Both  types  of  behavior  have  been  observed  in  some 
of  the  children  under  study.  Other  children  have  shown  a  third  danger 
signal-severe  and  protracted  "separation  anxiety"  when  the  mother  leaves 
the  room. 


The  Final  Subphase  of  Separation  Individuation 

The  fourth  subphase  begins  about  the  end  of  the  second  year  and 
continues  well  into  and  sometimes  all  the  way  through  the  third  year.  It  is 
a  complex  stage  which  the  researchers  describe  as  "the  period  in  which 
object  constancy  is  attained."  They  mean  by  this  that  during  the  final 
phase  of  the  separation-individuation  process  the  child  develops  the  ability 
to  retain  mental  representations  of  himself  and  his  mother  as  distinctly 

217 


separate  individuals.  When  she  is  absent,  the  investigators  explain,  he  win 
picture  her  as  being  away  from  him  and  also  as  returning  to  him;  lici 
continual  physical  presence  becomes  less  impeiative. 

As  an  indication  that  this  stage  has  begun,  the  child  makes  his  way  to 
the  toddler  room  For  prolonged  peiiods  and  accepts  the  nursery  teacher  us 
a  partial  substitute  for  his  mother.  For  example,  he  takes  food  irom  lit'f 
and  he  gladly  joins  with  her  in  play.  But  under  many  circumstances 
including  aggiession  by  or  against  another  child,  weariness,  a  bump,  soiled 
diapers-he  calls  for  his  mother  and  seeks  her  out.  And  if  he  is  in  that  kind 
of  emotional  state  he  will  insistently  demand  her  when  someone  else 
enters  the  room,  perhaps  the  opening  dooi  evokes  the  anticipation  ot  the 
mother's  appearance,  the  research  team  suggests,  or  perhaps  the  stress  ol 
having  to  cope  with  an  additional,  less  familiar  person  makes  the  need  for 
mother  more  acute. 

The  child  shows  an  inci easing  inteiest  both  in  his  playmates  itntl  in 
adults  other  than  his  mother.  He  is  mildly  or  moderately  ncg«livis!ic, 
showing  resistance  quite  often  to  the  demands  of  adults-a  cluiracterMk1, 
remarks  Dr.  Mahler,  that  seems  essential  for  the  development  of  a  sense-  of 
identity.  He  begins  to  develop  an  awareness  of  time,  commonly  itbsenl  in 
the  schizophrenic  child.  Along  with  it  he  shows  an  increased  nbilily  tn 
endure  separation  and  delays.  Concepts  like  "later"  and  "tomorrow11 
generally  first  associated  with  the  activities  of  his  mother-come  lo  !H! 
used  as  well  as  understood. 

Crises  arise  in  this  period,  Dr.  Mahler  reports,  when  the  mother  cannot 
accept  the  child's  negativistic  behavior  and  his  more  or  less  frequent 
display  of  "primary  process"  type  of  thinking,  manifested  in  primitive, 
illogical  talk  and  actions,  and  seen  at  its  most  extieme  in  schizophrenics. 
This  kind  of  thinking  is  common  among  youngsters  during  this  period, 
says  Dr.  Mahler,  but  even  some  apparently  normal  mothers  are  unable  lo 
deal  with  it  calmly  and  help  the  child  progress  to  the  rational  "secondary 
process."  One  mother,  for  example,  found  fault  with  her  youngster  for 
eating  well  at  Grandma's  but  poorly  at  home.  Angeicd,  the  child  toUl  her, 
"I  eat  you  up.  Then  I  spit  you  out.  Then  I  put  you  in  the  garbage  puil," 
The  mother  was  hurt,  angry,  and  disturbed.  She  and  the  yaimgsU-r 
bickered  for  an  hour.  Finally  the  girl  said,  "Then  I  put  you  back  loj'elher 
again."  It  would  have  been  more  conducive  to  emotional  health,  Dr. 
Mahler  observes,  had  the  mother  said  at  the  very  beginning  something 
like:  "Now  you  know  you  are  just  being  angry  at  Mommies,  Miiyto:  I 
shouldn't  have  said  anything  about  how  well  you  eat  at  Grandma's,  Any- 
way, you  know  very  well  that  nobody  eats  people  and  puts  them  in 
garbage  pails." 

Bridging  the  communicative  gap  between  the  child's  world  tmd  Ihe 
adult's,  says  the  investigator,  requires  the  deciphering  of  the  child's 
primary-process  language  and  actions,  playing  along  with  them,  ami 
gradually  offering  him  more  logical  expressions  and  more  rculislic 
solutions  to  his  problems.  Fortunately,  most  mothers  do  this.  To  find  and 
help  those  who  don't,  Dr.  Mahler  and  her  associates  believe,  would  be  one 
ot  the  ways  to  help  reduce  the  incidence  of  emotional  disorders. 

218 


Variations  in  Behavior 

The  types  of  behavior  characteristic  of  one  subphase  are  not  limited  to 
it.  In  other  periods,  though,  says  the  research  team,  either  they  are  seen 
less  frequently  or  they  are  less  important  to  the  particular  developmental 
tasks  with  which  the  child  is  dealing.  For  example,  a  joyful  exploration  of 
the  woild  is  characteristic  of  the  second,  or  practicing,  subphase  but  of 
course  occurs  during  the  rapprochement  period  as  well.  Its  occurrence 
during  that  period,  though,  is  less  significant  than  the  behavior  by  which 
the  child  is  working  through  the  job  of  finding  himself  an  individual, 
separate  from  his  mother,  yet  renewing  on  a  more  advanced  emotional 
level  his  bond  to  her  (rapprochement). 

Again,  during  the  final  subphase,  even  a  youngster  who  has  been  eagerly 
coming  to  the  toddler  room  for  a  long  time  may  suddenly  start  clinging  to 
his  mother  when  she  leaves  for  the  infant  room,  where  mothers  are  en- 
couraged to  stay.  Or  he  may  let  her  go  but  keep  running  back  and  forth, 
as  he  did  for  a  time  months  earlier. 

Nor  does  every  child  follow  the  pattern  most  characteristic  of  a 
particular  stage.  For  some  children  in  the  rapprochement  period,  the  need 
to  assert  separateness  over-shadows  the  need  to  establish  closeness.  While 
one  child  seeks  out  his  mother  with  every  new  toy  or  activity  in  order  to 
engage  her  attention  and  participation,  the  researchers  report,  another 
child  may  be  primarily  engaged  in  making  sure  that  this  very  thing  does 
not  happen.  He  will  hold  a  toy  in  his  hand,  look  at  his  mother,  and  veer 
off  to  seek  out  a  different  adult,  or  to  remain  by  himself.  This  is  most 
likely  to  be  so  if  his  mother  has  been  overprotective  and  intrusive;  in  this 
case  it  is  she,  rather  than  the  child,  who  becomes  the  "shadower." 


The  Mother's  Influence 

The  research  is  documenting  what  most  mothers  know-that  different 
children  do  not  proceed  through  a  given  development  stage  either  at  the 
same  rate  or  in  the  same  way.  And  it  is  uncovering  evidence  that  the 
motheis  themselves  influence  the  pattern. 

Early  during  the  first  stage  of  the  separalion-individimtion  process,  for 
example,  Bernie  showed  great  interest  in  locomotion  and  would  per- 
sistently try  to  crawl  and  to  pull  himself  up.  Stuart,  on  the  other  hand, 
usually  just  lay  on  the  floor  and  looked;  though  obviously  interested  in 
the  things  and  the  people  around  him,  he  made  little  effoit  to  get  to  them. 
Constitutional  factors  may  have  been  at  work,  say  the  investigators,  but  so 
were  the  attitudes  of  the  mothers.  Bernie's  mother  often  appeared  listless 
and  apathetic  during  the  earliest  months  but  cheered  up—and  encouraged 
the  boy-when  he  began  moving  around.  Stuart's  mother,  though,  liked  to 
keep  him  close.  Instead  of  encouraging  him  to  shift  for  himself,  she  met 
all  his  needs  so  promptly  that  he  never  had  to  exert  himself  to  get  what  he 
wanted.  On  the  developmental  tests  when  they  first  came  to  the  center, 

219 


Bernie  and  Stuart  rated  about  the  same.  A  few  years  later,  however,  Stuart 
did  not  do  nearly  so  well  as  Bernie. 

Two  sisters,  Ann  and  Susan,  are  offered  as  particularly  good  illustra- 
tions of  the  strength  of  the  maternal  influence  during  the  separation- 
individuation  phase. 

When  Ann  was  9  months  old,  she  was  often  observed  sitting  at  her 
mother's  feet,  looking  up  at  her  and  patiently  begging  for  attention.  She 
got  very  little  of  it  and  in  consequence,  the  investigators  say,  had  little 
psychic  energy  for  investing  In  the  activities  normal  to  the  next,  or 
practicing,  subphase.  She  would  make  only  brief  forays  from  her  mother; 
her  "love  affair  with  the  world"  was  subdued  and  of  unusually  short 
duration.  Throughout  the  final  sub  phase,  Ann  was  an  unhappy  little  girl 
who  could  not  easily  endure  physical  separation  from  her  mother,  did  not 
get  along  well  with  other  adults  and  children,  yet  showed  little  joy  when 
the  mother  returned  after  brief,  everyday  separations.  In  one  camera- 
recorded  scene,  she  has  a  tantrum  when  her  mother  starts  to  leave  the 
room;  the  child  insists  on  going  along  but  then  gives  up  and  just  stands 
there,  suffering.  Finally  she  regresses  by  retiring  to  the  play  area  for  the 
youngest  babies.  She  turns  her  back  on  the  other  people  and  is  clearly 
hurt  and  angry, 

Like  a  few  of  the  other  children  in  the  study,  Ann  is  described  as 
already  vulnerable— already  in  trouble.  The  investigators  believe  that  these 
vulnerable  children,  unless  further  environmental  experience  amply 
compensates,  may  well  develop  emotional  problems  rooted  in  the  unmet 
needs  and  unaccomplished  tasks  of  the  separation-Individ  nation  phase. 
After  the  present  study  has  been  completed,  the  researchers  hope  to 
predict  what  types  of  problems  are  likely  to  develop  in  a  given  child  under 
given  circumstances  and  then  to  keep  in  touch  with  each  family  foi  some 
years. 

With  Susan,  Ann's  younger  sister,  the  mother  had  mellowed  even 
though  she  was  the  same  somewhat  aloof,  self-centered  person.  At  the 
center,  every  so  often  she  would  put  the  baby  down  and  bury  herself  in 
the  newspaper.  But  Susan  was  a  more  outward -going  and  determined  baby 
than  Ann,  and  when  she  wanted  her  mother's  attention,  she  knew  how  to 
go  about  getting  it.  In  one  scene,  she  tugs  at  her  mother's  dress,  beseeches 
her  with  her  eyes,  and  finally  starts  to  pull  herself  up  to  her  mother's 
knee.  The  viewer  can  almost  hear  the  mother  say,  "Oh,  the  heck  with  it," 
as  she  puts  the  paper  aside  and  lovingly  picks  up  the  baby.  In  a  later 
subphase,  Susan  looks  distressed  when  her  mother  leaves  the  room  but, 
unlike  her  sister,  soon  turns  happily  to  playing  with  the  other  adults  find 
children.  She  is  joyful  when  her  mother  comes  back.  A  child  who  has  a 
good  relationship  with  her  mother,  Dr.  Mahler  emphasizes,  shows  relative- 
ly little  separation  anxiety. 

The  difference  between  Ann's  experiences  and  Susan's,  the  investigators 
suggest,  may  have  had  a  genetic  basis,  since  the  younger  girl  was  able  to 
command  the  mother's  attention  and  draw  upon  and  be  secure  in  her  love 
while  the  other  was  not.  But  they  believe  that  Ann's  progress  through  this 

220 


critical  period  would  have  been  less  stormy  with  a  mother  moie  attuned 
to  the  child's  emotional  needs. 

With  Genie,  Danny,  and  Matthew— three  children  of  Mrs.  A.,  all  of 
whom  have  been  studied  at  the  center— the  similarities  have  been  more 
striking  than  the  differences.  All  three  have  been  conspicuously  motor 
minded.  They  undoubtedly  had  an  inborn  motor  proclivity,  says  Dr. 
Mahler,  but  they  also  had  a  powerful  secondaiy  impetus:  the  mother's 
great  interest  in  having  them  become  independent  as  soon  as  possible.  The 
incessant  crawling,  climbing,  seesaw  balancing,  and  such  activities  were 
not  only  pleasant  in  themselves  but  also  brought  the  mother's  approval. 
Particularly  in  Genie's  case,  the  investigator  believes  that  the  hyperactivity 
probably  served  another  purpose  as  well:  it  seemed  to  make  up  for  an 
unmet  need  arising  from  the  mother's  tendency  to  ward  off  physical 
closeness  and  cuddling. 

"The  research  has  clearly  established,"  Dr.  Mahler  reports  in  summing 
up  the  work  so  far,  "that  the  emotional  availability  of  the  mother  is 
necessary  for  the  optimal  unfolding  of  the  child's  innate  potentialities." 
This  is  so,  she  explains,  because  the  child,  throughout  the  process  of 
separating  from  the  mother,  is  emotionally  so  dependent  upon  her.  If  he  is 
left  markedly  unceitain  about  her  emotional  availability,  signs  of  potential 
trouble  aieseen. 

Dr.  Mahler  adds  that  the  research  also  points  to  the  sturdiness  and 
adaptive  capacity  of  the  normal  toddler.  "Even  against  considerable 
odds,"  she  remarks,  "he  is  usually  able  to  extract  from  his  mother  the 
necessary  emotional  supplies." 

When  the  cunent  work  has  been  completed,  the  investigators  expect  to 
write  a  detailed  description  of  mother-child  interactions  during  each  part 
of  the  separation-individuation  phase  and  to  list  and  explain  the  danger 
signs—those  behaviors  and  attitudes  believed  to  warn  of  impending 
emotional  trouble  for  the  child  unless  corrected. 


Research  Grants:  MH  8238,  Mil  3353 
Date  of  Intei  view:  Mar.  17,  1966 

References: 

Mahler,  Margaret  S.  Thoughts  about  development  and  individuation.  Psychoanal.  Stud, 

Child,  1963,  18. 
Mahler,    Margaret    S.    On    early    infantile   psychosis:    the   symbiotic   and    autistic 

syndromes./  Amer.  Acad,  Child  Psychiat.,  1965,4,4. 
Mahler,  Margaret  S.  On  the  significance  of  the  normal  separation-individuation  phase, 

Drives,  Affects,  Behav,,  1965,  2. 
Mahler,  Margaret  S.  On  the  development  of  basic  moods,  and  the  depressive  effect.  In 

Solnit,  Schur  &  Loewenstein  (Eds.),  Heinz  Hartmann  Festschrift.  New  York;  Inter- 
national Universities  Press,  1966. 
Mahler,  Margaret  S.,  Purer,  M.,  &  Settlage,  C.  FF.  Severe  emotional  disturbances  in 

childhood;  psychosis.  In  American   handbook  of  psychiatry.   New  York:  Basic 

Books,  1959. 
Mahler,  Margaret  S.f  &  Laperriere,  Kitty.  Mother-child  interaction  during  separation 

individuation.  Psychoanal  Quart.,  1965,34. 


221 


Investigator: 

Richard  Q.  Bell,  Ph.D., 
Chief,  Infant  Development  Section 
Child  Research  Branch,  NIMH 

Prepared  by: 

Antoinette  Gattozzi 


Most  psychologists  would  agree  that  social  behavior  develops  out  oflh: 
interplay  between  the  individual  and  his  environment.  From  birth  to  <MtK 
childhood  the  most  significant  elements  of  the  individual's  environmcnl 
are  probably  his  parents.  The  relationship  between  child  and  ptircnls.ontt 
thought  to  fit  a  simple  stimulus-response  paradigm,  generally  is  rc^rited 
today  as  a  complex  interaction  whose  nature  is  affected  by  both  child  ar.J 
parents.  The  contributions  of  each  coalesce  with  gieat  subllely  iintl  be- 
come more  and  moie  difficult  to  distinguish  as  the  relationship  mmi> 
through  time,  In  a  research  piogram  designed  to  circumvent  thisdifficu! 
ty,  Dr.  Richard  Q.  Bell  and  his  associates  are  looking  for  newborn  hchavicr 
characteristics  that  are  precursors  of  social  development  at  Ihe  prcschorf 
age.  The  investigators  are  studying  infants  before  they  begin  to  inters! 
with  parents,  then  observing  the  subjects'  social  behavior  when  they  rva^ 
the  age  of  2/2  years. 

Dr.  Bell  became  interested  in  the  direction  of  effects  at  work  h 
parent-infant  interaction  tlirough  research  he  did  some  time  iigo  with  Or 
Earl  S.  Schaeffer  of  the  NIMH  Laboratory  of  Psychology.  Tim  luo 
scientists  devised  a  questionnaire  to  compare  attitudes  of  molhm  of 
mentally  normal  and  abnormal  children.  Attitude  patterns  of  mo  liters  of 
schizophrenic  children  could  not  be  distinguished  from  those  of  niolhwi 
whose  children  were  mentally  retarded  because  of  brain  damage  orotrm 
birth  defects.  Dr.  Bell  inferred  that  the  same  factor-reaction  to  the  diiU'i 
condition-might  be  the  significant  one.  This  line  of  reasoning  led  hiniK 
questions  about  the  child's  effect  on  the  parent  and  to  the  search  f« 
congenital  characteristics. 

Increasingly  in  recent  years,  clinicians  have  postulated  tluit  faclon 
contributing  to  temperament  differences  among  individuals  m;iy  te 
present  at  birth,  and  that  these  early  differences  may  be  implicated  in  to 
later  development  of  mental  illnesses.  No  clear  evidence  has  been  four-J 
however,  that  indicates  what  these  factors  are  or  how  they  are  dGlckriQu* 
The  investigators'  efforts  to  identify  congenital  contributions  to  SOCL! 
development,  although  not  directly  concerned  with  etiological  problem*. 
may  show  how  to  begin  to  answer  such  questions. 


Two  aspects  of  behavioral  development  were  of  special  interest  to  the 
investigators— the  child's  tendency  to  stay  near  adults  and  his  ability  to 
cope  with  sudden,  unexpected  changes  in  the  environment.  Attachment  to 
adults,  in  a  sense,  is  the  binding  force  of  the  parent-child  interaction  as 
well  as  its  most  important  product.  The  mother  may  have  no  task  of 
gi eater  consequence  during  the  first  few  months  of  her  infant's  life  than 
to  make  her  presence  and  actions  important  to  him.  Both  attachment  to 
adults  and  coping  ability  are  highly  salient  features  of  human  behavior, 
the  development  of  which  can  be  observed  and  measured  in  very  young 
children.  Moreover,  these  behaviors  show  a  wide  range  of  variability  in  any 
random  sample  of  childien.  "They  are  fundamental  dimensions  of  normal 
development,"  Dr.  Bell  notes,  "and  they  find  a  prominent  place  in 
descriptions  of  both  normal  and  aberrant  personalities." 

The  research  proceeded  in  two  parallel  projects.  In  one,  the  scientists 
observed  neonates  to  record  characteristics  of  ^individual  infants  before 
cultural  forces  had  begun  to  shape  them.  These  children  were  studied  again 
when  they  reached  preschool  age.  In  the  other  project,  the  investigators 
observed  the  play  of  preschool  children  to  detect  patterns  of  social  attach- 
ment and  coping  ability.  Questionnaires  were  used  in  both  projects.  Each 
mother  of  a  newborn  subject  filled  in  a  questionnahe  about  her  infant's 
behavior  when  he  was  1  month  old,  then  completed  the  same  question- 
naire when  he  was  about  2'/2  yeais  old.  Mothers  of  the  preschool  children 
not  studied  as  newborns  completed  the  same  questionnaire;  that  is,  each 
recalled  her  child's  behavior  as  an  infant.  Dr.  Bell's  design  of  the  research 
included  both  prospective  observations  and  retrospective  questionnaiies 
because  he  hoped  to  demonstrate  how  the  two  approaches  might  be  com- 
bined in  a  longitudinal  study  to  accelerate  data  analysis.  This  is  another  of 
Dr.  Bell's  research  interests;  it  is  a  central  concern  in  difficult,  time- 
consuming  longitudinal  studies. 

The  major  outcome  of  the  program  to  date  is  the  tentative 
identification  of  a  congenital  behavior  syndrome,  a  pattern  of  newborn 
behavior  that  is  linked  to  a  pattern  of  social  behavior  at  age  2V4  years,  The 
investigators  found  that  a  newborn  male  who  shows  high  formula 
consumption  relative  to  birth  weight,  whose  respiration  is  rapid,  and 
whose  mother  describes  him  as  restless  and  hard  to  soothe  at  age  1  month, 
will  be,  at  the  age  of  2Vfc  years,  relatively  nonsocial.  He  will  be  rather 
disinterested  in  other  children  and  relatively  independent  of  adults. 

Research  results  also  include,  in  brief,  the  discovery  of  biological 
consequences  associated  with  birth  order  and  sex,  the  identification  of 
biological  effects  of  family  size  and  structure  on  preschool  social  behavior, 
and  the  establishment  of  a  tentative  link  between  patterns  of  preschool 
social  behavior  and  styles  of  cognition  at  age  6  years. 


The  Study  of  Newborns 

Newborn  infants  are  notably  difficult  subjects  to  study.  In  the  welter  of 
behavior  they  exhibit,  many  items  are  affected  by  temporary  conditions 

223 


of  this  period.  Above  all,  change  in  the  newborn  is  swift.  To 
meaningful  and  replicable  issults,  factors  known  to  affect  neoi 
behavior  tiansiently  either  must  be  accounted  for  or  their  eff 
eliminated  from  the  data.  Elements  that  change  rapidly  have  tc 
avoided.  Dr.  Bell  comments:  "As  an  uncivilized  human  being  the  newt 
is  intriguing  theoretically,  while  being  at  the  same  time  one  of  the  r 
difficult  subjects  as  far  as  abstracting  stable  measurements  is  concert!' 
Many  past  studies  of  newboins  reported  conflicting  or  inconck 
results  because  they  failed  to  isolate  influential  variables.  Dr. 
compiled  the  following  list  of  factors  to  be  controlled  in  studies  of  inf 
and  young  children.  It  leveals  how  demanding  such  research  is,  espec 
studies  carried  out  during  the  first  hundred  horns  of  life. 

1 .  State  of  arousal—  There  is  a  continuum  of  aiousal  that  ranges  I 
quiet  sleep  at  one  end  to  the  excitation  of  robust  crying  at  the  other 
The  infant's  position   along  the  quiescent  range  of  the  continuui 
particularly  difficult  to  judge,  yet  his  arousal  state  clearly  affects 
behavior. 

2.  Complications   of  pregnancy   and   delivery.- These    exert    n 
effects,  depending  on  their  severity.  In  this  connection  it  is  worth  nc 
that  complications  are  significantly  fewer  the  higher  the  socioecom 
level,  and  that  males  are  more  subject  to  complications  than  females. 

3.  Parity. -Evidence     exists     both     documenting     certain     tran 
congenital  differences  between  fiist-and  later-born  children  and  hinti 
the  possibility  of  lasting  differences.  For  example,  first-borns  have 
shown  to  have  higher  levels  of  L-lactic  acid  and  1 7-hydroxy  corticoste 
during  the  first  few  days  of  life. 

4.  Analgesics  and  anesthetics.-ThQsz  effects  may   originate   in 
prenatal  or  postnatal  periods.  Barbiturate  medication  give  predeliver) 
instance,  has  a  pronounced  dampening  effect  on  the  newborn's  fei 
adequacy  and  attention  to  visual  stimuli  up  to  the  fourth  day  of  li 
mother  who  is  breast  feeding  and  on  sedation  may  pass  residual  sed 
to  her  infant. 

5.  Age  and  sex-Theie  are  key  parameters  in  themselves,  as  w< 
factors  that  interact  with  others. 

Over  a  period  of  2  years,  the  investigators  studied  a  total  of  75  in 
in  the  nursery  of  Suburban  Hospital  in  Bethesda,  Md.  All  infants 
white,  full-term,  and  free  of  complications  of  pregnancy  and  birth,  s 
as  their  records  indicated.  They  came  from  families  of  various  s 
economic  backgrounds,  and  none  was  a  first  child.  Observations 
made  on  the  third  and  fourth  days  of  life-as  far  as  possible  fron 
confounding  conditions  concomitant  with  birth  and  as  close  as  possil 
the  time  infants  were  to  be  taken  home.  Among  the  measures  tl 
searchers  obtained  were  rate  and  regularity  of  respiration,  mouth  i 
ments,  eye  movements  behind  closed  lids  during  sleep,  rate  and  pattc 
non-nutritive  sucking  (on  a  sterile  nipple  stuffed  with  cotton  and  of 
ju&t  before  regular  feeding),  rapidity  and  vigor  of  response  to  in  term 
of  feeding,  amount  of  time  and  height  to  which  an  infant  raised  his 
while  prone,  tactile  sensitivity,  and  various  body  dimensions.  Of  t 

224 


neonate  characteristics  measured,  the  investigators  were  interested  only  in 
those  that  showed  wide  range  across  infants  and  stability  within  individual 
infants,  or  those  linked  to  known  factors  such  as  sex  or  parity.  Stability 
was  judged  by  the  consistency  of  the  measure  from  test  to  retest. 

Interaction  between  sex  and  type  of  feeding.  -Measures  were  made  of 
the  prone  head  reaction  (PHR),  which  is  presumed  to  be  a  correlate  of 
muscle  strength.  The  infant  was  placed  supine  for  55  seconds,  then  turned 
to  the  prone  position.  A  soft  rubber  device  was  held  near  the  infant's  head 
to  measure  how  high  the  chin  was  lifted  during  the  next  60  seconds.  On 
the  basis  of  past  studies,  the  researchers  expected  males  to  show  higher 
PHR  scores  than  females,  indicating  gi  eater  muscle  strength  in  males. 
They  also  expected  that  PHR  would  correlate  negatively  with  skin 
sensitivity,  which  was  being  tested  separately. 

Results  were  no  more  than  suggestive  concerning  the  hypothesis  of 
greater  muscle  strength  in  males,  and  the  data  relating  muscle  strength  to 
skin  sensitivity  also  were  inconclusive.  Although  the  association  was  in  the 
expected  direction— the  greater  the  muscle  strength,  the  lower  the  sensi- 
tivity of  the  skin— correlations  did  not  reach  significance. 

Perhaps  the  most  valuable  result  of  the  PHR  measurements  was  the 
discovery  of  a  transient  interaction  between  sex  and  type  of  feeding.  On 
the  first  round  of  measurements,  breast-fed  males  and  bottle-fed  females 
scored  the  highest  PHR  values;  13  hours  later,  at  the  time  of  the  second 
observation  cycle,  this  phenomenon  receded  and  males  scored  generally 
higher  than  females  regardless  of  the  way  they  were  fed. 

The  investigators  found  the  same  interaction  between  sex  and  type  of 
feeding  in  their  study  of  tactile  sensitivity.  This  was  not  unexpected  in 
view  of  the  relation,  even  though  it  is  of  low  order,  between  muscle 
strength  and  skin  sensitivity.  Unlike  the  experience  with  the  PHR 
measures,  however,  the  effect  of  the  interaction  did  not  disappear  on 
retest.  Breast-fed  males  and  bottle-fed  females  still  showed  less  skin 
sensitivity.  These  results  confirmed  the  finding  that  type  of  feeding  can 
interact  with  sex  to  produce  an  effect  during  the  neonate  period;  the 
interaction  affects,  at  least,  PHR  and  skin  sensitivity,  Studies  of  newborns 
that  unknowingly  sampled  different  proportions  of  breast-fed  males  and 
bottle-fed  females  could  encounter  contradictory  data. 

Yet  another  surprising  effect  related  to  type  of  feeding  was  noted, 
After  infants  were  handled  for  body  measurements,  breast-fed  babies 
became  and  remained  more  active  than  bottle-fed  infants.  Dr.  Bell  offered 
two  possible  explanations.  An  electrolytic  difference  between  the  two 
kinds  of  milk  may  differentially  affect  arousal  centers;  or  perhaps  a 
breast-fed  infant  is  more  responsive  to  handling  because  he  is  usually  more 
hungry  than  one  fed  by  bottle.  This  may  occur  because  his  mother's  milk 
is  low  in  volume  or  late  in  starting. 

Relation  of  skin  sensitivity  and  arousal  characteristics.— The  original 
impetus  behind  the  study  of  skin  sensitivity  was  to  search  for  sex  dif- 
ferences, There  is  evidence  that  adult  females  have  greater  skin  sensitivity 
than  adult  males.  Such  reports  contributed  to  a  hypothesis  developed 
from  observations  of  primitive  human  societies.  The  hypothesis  is  that 

225 


males  are  equipped  congenially  with  gi  eater  muscle  strength  and  higlu 
tactile  thresholds  than  females  in  order  to  fulfill  the  adult  male  role  c 
gross  motility  in  space  and  low  response  to  pain.  The  principal  adul 
female  role,  on  the  other  hand,  is  the  place-bound  care  of  offspring,  <i  rol 
enhanced,  according  to  the  hypothesis,  by  the  female's  lesser  muscl 
strength  and  higher  skin  sensitivity 

Investigations  of  newborn  skin  sensitivity  have  not  been  able  toga  the 
definitive  evidence  demonstrating  a  sex  difference.  It  is  still  not  known 
therefore,  to  what  extent  the  greatei  skin  sensitivity  of  adult  females  is  j 
congenital  attribute  and  in  what  degree  it  is  culturally  induced.  Tc 
discover  how  nature  and  nurture  interact,  scientists  must  isolate  anc 
measure  each  influence  separately. 

The  investigatois  tried  three  different  kinds  of  skin  stimuli:  Removal  oi 
a  coveiing  blanket,  a  fine  jet  of  air,  and  application  of  the  nylon  points  ol 
an  instrument  known  as  an  aesthesiometer.  The  first  two  of  the  three  test; 
confirmed  the  hypothesis;  their  results  pointed  to  greater  skin  sensitivity 
in  females  than  in  males.  Females  were  more  sensitive  to  stimulation  ol 
the  abdomen,  for  example,  than  were  males.  The  interaction  of  sex  and 
type  of  feeding  was  found  in  the  trials  using  the  aesthesiometer. 

The  procedure  with  the  aesthesiometer  followed  this  form.  As  the 
infant  slept,  one  of  a  set  of  increasingly  thick  nylon  filaments  was  pressed 
against  the  heel  of  the  left  foot  until  the  filament  bent,  at  which  point  (he 
pressure  being  exerted  was  exactly  known.  When  the  pressure  of  a 
filament  elicited  a  response— anything  from  the  flexing  of  two  toes  lo 
mass  body  movement-the  value  was  recorded  as  threshold.  The  procedure 
was  followed  three  to  five  times  duiing  a  sleep  cycle  (the  infants  slept 
right  through  the  tests),  and  the  final  score  was  the  mean  of  the  individual 
trials. 

As  noted  above,  the  tactile  sensitivity  data  showed  sex  diffeiences  in 
some  tests  and  on  some  subjects  but  not  others,  and  the  question  of 
congenital  sex  diffeiences  in  skin  sensitivity  could  not  be  settled.  How- 
ever, two  consistent  relations  appeared  that  opened  an  avenue  for  follow- 
up  studies.  Skin  sensitivity  scores  were  found  to  relate  to  other  charac- 
teristics of  the  infants,  Newborns  with  low  skin  sensitivity  showed  a  low 
level  of  arousal  as  indicated  by  lespiration  characteristics  while  asleep  and 
by  slow  response  to  interruption  of  sucking.  Thus  the  data  contributed  to 
one  of  the  research  program's  preliminary  goals,  the  description  of  new- 
born behavior  patterns  that  might  be  related  to  later  behavior  patterns, 

Effects  associated  with  sex  and  parity.— \l  became  obvious  early  in  the 
course  of  the  newborn  study  that  precise  location  of  an  infant's  position 
on  the  aiousal  continuum  was  essential  to  clarify  other  variables.  Arousal 
levels  were  monitored  by  subjective  measures  when  the  infant  was  awake 
and  by  respiration  counts  when  asleep.  The  investigators  were  not  entirely 
satisfied  with  the  subjective  measures  and  their  use  made  replication  of 
the  study  by  others  hard  to  do.  To  find  out  whether  an  objective  measure 
of  arousal  might  be  used  instead,  Dr.  George  M.  Weller  undertook  a 
separate  study  of  infants'  galvanic  skin  response.  (Dr.  Weller  was  a  Ph.  P. 

226 


candidate  working  at  the  Child  Research  Blanch  at  the  time.  He  is  now  on 
the  faculty  of  Temple  University  in  Philadelphia.) 

Forty  newborn  infants  from  the  nursery  of  the  U.S.  Naval  Hospital 
Medical  Center  in  Bethesda  were  the  subjects.  They  were  screened  to 
exclude  any  with  prenatal  or  perinatal  complications.  Only  bottle-fed 
infants  were  chosen,  and  both  first-  and  later-born  were  included.  Skin 
conductance  was  measured  by  an  electrode  disc  taped  to  the  bottom  of 
each  foot.  Activity  was  observed  by  Dr.  Weller  and  recoided  on  a  manual 
counter.  Some  items  in  the  activity  inventory  were  agitated  crying,  head 
movements,  arm  and  hand  movements,  movements  of  the  eyes  behind 
closed  lids,  and  no  movement.  These  items  defined  six  states  of  arousal 
langing  from  deep  sleep  to  marked  agitation.  Visual  counts  of  respirations 
were  made  and  then  galvanic  response  was  measured. 

Dr.  Weller  found  conductance  and  activity  levels  positively  correlated 
more  strongly  in  the  waking  than  sleeping  states;  respiration,  too,  after 
special  statistical  treatment  of  the  data,  showed  a  positive  correlation  with 
conductance.  The  lesults  indicated  that  galvanic  skin  icsponse  might  be 
used  as  an  objective  measure  of  infant  arousal  levels.  Dr.  Weller  also 
discovered  that  females  and  later-born  infants  showed  consistently  higher 
skin  conductance  and  displayed  highei  levels  of  activity  than  their 
counterparts.  He  cautioned  that  before  any  implications  may  be  drawn, 
the  findings  must  first  be  confirmed  (the  finding  on  females  has  since  been 
located  in  data  fiom  another  study),  and  then  shown  to  extend  beyond 
the  first  days  of  life.  Nevertheless,  the  study  found  the  first  unequivocal 
evidence  of  congenital  differences  between  males  and  females  and 
between  first-  and  later-born  infants.  Because  differences  in  conductance 
are  associated  with  differences  in  maturation  during  this  very  early  period, 
it  can  be  said  that  females  and  later-borns  arc  congenially  moie  mature 
than  their  counterparts  up  to  the  fourth  clay  of  life. 

The  results  may  be  pertinent  to  the  question  of  how  nature  and  nurture 
interact.  Girls  are  treated  differently  from  boys  by  theii  parents;  second- 
and  later-born  children  not  only  experience  the  company  of  siblings 
absent  from  a  first-born's  early  months  of  life,  they  also  get  different 
treatment  from  parents  now  versed  in  child  rearing,  These  cultural  differ- 
ences frequently  are  cited  to  explain  the  persistent  and  often  very  obvious 
differences  in  social  behavior  between  males  and  females  and  between 
first-  and  later-born  children.  Dr.  Welter's  findings  indicate  that  biological 
factors  may  play  a  role  in  these  differences. 

Stable  characteristics  of  newborns.-ln  the  end,  the  voluminous  data 
accumulated  in  the  newborn  study  were  analyzed  to  extract  items  that 
were  stable  from  test  to  retest.  Respiration  rate  during  sleep  and  tactile 
sensitivity  as  measured  by  the  aesthesiometer  were  the  most  stable  charac- 
teristics. One  other  measure  readily  available  from  hospital  records  proved 
very  stable;  this  was  the  amount  of  formula  consumed  by  bottle-fed 
infants.  Sucking  patterns,  total  crying  in  response  to  sucking  interruption, 
and  PHR  were  next  in  order  in  stability.  Borderline  stability  was  shown  by 
measures  of  respiration  variability,  pattern  of  crying,  and  reaction  time  in 


response  to  sucking  interruption.  It  seems  doubtful  that  many  measures  o 
arousal  level  and  spontaneous  skeletal  muscle  movements  will  be  useful  ii 
studies  of  differences  among  newboins  because  of  the  instability  o 
individual  scores  on  these  items. 


The  Study  of  Preschool  Children 

The  preliminary  study  of  preschool  social  behavior  began  with  sepaiati 
samples  of  2-year-old  boys  and  gnls  for  whom  no  newborn  data  wen 
available,  and  continued  adding  subjects  as  the  children  completed  the  I 
month  of  study  and  the  investigators  shaipened  their  methods.  Childrci 
selected  for  these  preliminaiy  studies  were  second-  or  later-borns.  Eacl 
mother  completed  a  questionnaire  that  asked  her  to  lecall  her  child1; 
behavior  as  an  infant.  From  this  questionnaire  the  researchers  drew  uj 
"clusters"  of  infant  behavior  characteristics.  These  recalled  chaiacteristic! 
were  then  lelated  to  clusters  of  preschool  behavior  displayed  by  tin 
children.  The  analysis  gave  the  investigators  their  first  leads  to  the  new- 
born and  preschool  relations  they  might  find  when  they  followed  up  the 
newborn  subjects. 

Later,  when  the  children  who  were  studied  as  newborns  were  near  the 
age  of  214  yeais,  they  were  brought  to  the  nursery  school  daily  in  groups 
of  six  or  seven  of  the  same  sex.  Just  as  in  the  preliminary  studies,  they 
were  studied  at  play  by  two  teachers  who  participated  in  their  activities 
and  by  two  observers  seated  behind  a  one-way  vision  window.  None  knew 
what  newborn  factois  had  been  singled  out  for  possible  relations  to  [he 
behavior  they  watched,  The  observers  counted  and  timed  aspects  of  play 
and  the  teachers  rated  the  children  on  such  items  as  amount  of  physical 
contact  initiated  with  teachers,  ability  to  be  soothed  by  teachers  when 
upset,  and  relations  with  peers.  Boys  and  girls  displayed  different  patterns 
of  play,  an  observation  made  by  many  other  students  of  child  behavior. 
Girls  were  more  aware  of  others  than  boys  and  were  more  shy  with  strange 
adults.  Boys  were  more  interested  in  toys  than  people.  Girls  seemed  more 
mature  than  boys  in  speech  development  and  motor  coordination. 

Maternal  recall  as  a  mediator  variable,  -Dr,  Bell  devised  a  complex  series 
of  analyses  employing  maternal  recall  as  a  mediator  variable  to  link  the 
prospective  and  retrospective  data  and  test  the  feasibility  of  this  method 
of  accelerated  longitudinal  analysis.  The  series  began,  as  noted  above,  wilh 
the  first  samples  of  the  preschool  study  by  testing  for  relationships 
between  sets  of  measures  made  by  observing  a  child's  behavior  in  nursery 
school  and  sets  derived  from  the  mother's  retrospective  report  of  his 
infancy.  The  comparison  yielded  the  first  potential  relations  between 
infancy  and  preschool  behavior, 

The  next  analysis  used  questionnaires  completed  when  their  infants 
were  1  month  of  age  by  mothers  of  subjects  studied  as  newborns.  Sets  of 
items  located  on  these  were  tested  for  relationships  with  sets  from  the 
(same)  questionnaire  retrospectively  completed  by  mothers  of  preschool 
subjects  not  studied  as  newborns.  In  this  way,  the  questionnaire  itself  was 

228 


validated  and  measures  were  located  in  the  1 -month  questionnaires  that 
could  be  related  to  measures  made  in  newborn  observations 

A  third  analysis  located  relations  between  sets  of  newborn  measures 
and  sets  of  items  from  questionnaires  completed  at  age  1  month.  The  final 
step  was  to  test  foi  relations  between  newborn  and  pieschool  chaiac- 
teristics  in  a  common  sample.  One  set  of  newborn  characteristics  does 
appear  to  be  associated  with  a  set  of  pieschool  social  characteristics.  This 
is  the  congenital  behavior  syndrome  in  males  discussed  earlier  in  this 
leport.  It  traces  continuity  between  newborn  behavior  and  social  lespon- 
siveness;  no  clear  relations  have  been  found  yet  between  newboin  charac- 
teristics and  later  coping  ability.  The  findings  that  led  to  the  description 
of  the  congenital  behavior  syndrome  cuirently  are  being  prepared  for 
publication,  along  with  a  report  of  relations  found  between  all  measures. 
The  latter  analyses  do  not  rely  on  mediator  variables  such  as  maternal 
report  or  an  index  of  family  size  and  structuie.  This  index,  in  addition  to 
its  potentiality  as  a  mediator  variable,  was  found  to  be  related  to  aspects 
of  dependency  behavior. 

Effects  of  family  size  and  stmcture.-hi\  arresting  featuie  of  behavior 
had  appeared  in  the  preliminary  sample  of  preschoolers.  Boys  from  large 
families  with  short  intervals  between  children  were  consistently  higher 
than  other  boys  on  the  rating  of  teacher  contact.  Mrs.  Mary  F.  Waldrop, 
director  of  the  nursery  school  and  one  of  the  teacheis,  explored  this 
obseivation.  She  noted  studies  of  young  animals  made  elsewhere  that 
leported  an  offspring's  need  for  maternal  physical  contact.  Other  re- 
searchers showed  that  a  child  depiived  of  protective  adult  support  will 
increase  his  dependency  behavior  when  he  does  have  contact  with  adults. 
These  reports  suggested  to  Mrs.  Waldrop  that  a  child  has  a  need  for 
physical  contact  with  a  mother  (or  a  mother  substitute  such  as  a  female 
teacher)  and  that  if  the  need  is  not  met,  he  increases  his  efforts  to  achieve 
physical  contact.  A  mother  of  many  children  closely  spaced  cannot  always 
be  available  to  any  one  child;  indeed,  other  investigators  have  found  that 
mothers  of  many  children  tend  to  become  unconcerned  about  the 
younger  ones.  With  these  clues,  Mrs.  Wakhop  began  an  analysis  to  test  the 
hypothesis  that  preschool  boys  from  large  families  would  show  more 
contact-seeking  behavior  with  a  female  teacher  than  boys  of  the  same  age 
from  smaller  families.  Further,  by  using  some  information  about  maternal 
availability  in  the  home,  she  hoped  to  get  a  lead  on  whether  a  huge  family 
was  associated  with  low-maternal  availability. 

The  first  step  was  to  quantify  the  size  and  structure  of  a  child's  family 
in  an  index  of  family  size  and  density  (FSD)  composed  of  four  weighted 
factors.  These  were  number  of  children,  timespan  between  the  child  and 
his  next  younger  sibling,  timespan  between  the  child  and  his  next  older 
sibling,  and  average  timespan  between  all  births.  The  higher  the  FSD 
index,  the  larger  and  more  dense  was  the  family  into  which  the  child  was 
bom. 

An  FSD  index  was  computed  for  each  of  44  boys  and  compared  with 
the  rating  given  him  on  the  behavior  "child  initiated  contact  with  female 
teacher."  The  postulated  relation  emerged.  Moreover,  a  prediction  based 

229 


on  an  alternative  hypothesis  was  not  suppoitcd.  The  behavior  could  no 
be  explained  by  a  child's  greater  experience  of  social  contacts  because  o 
the  number  of  siblings  he  had:  There  was  no  correlation  between  FSD  an< 
the  rating  "friendliness  with  peers." 

A  significant  negative  correlation  was  found  between  FSD  and  a  ratiiij 
made  in  the  child's  home  of  maternal  initiation  of  contact.  This  findinj 
raised  the  possibility  that  low-maternal  availability  in  large,  dense  families 
may  be  a  factoi  in  the  heightened  contact-seeking  behavior  of  childici 
born  into  these  families. 

Are  there  other  factors  that  might  cause  such  a  child  to  seek  mow 
contact  with  adults  than  do  other  children?  Phrasing  the  question  in  the 
investigators'  teims,  aie  such  children  congenitally  different  from  children 
born  into  small,  low-density  families? 

To  answer  this  question,  Mrs.  Waldrop  turned  to  the  analysis  of  a 
different  set  of  data  that  had  just  become  available.  This  was  a  sample  of 
boys  and  giils  for  whom  both  newborn  and  nursery  school  measures  were 
available.  From  the  newborn  data  the  investigator  constructed  a  lethargy 
scoie  for  each  child.  It  was  obtained  by  adding  the  infant's  ranks  on  (1) 
suck  rate,  the  number  of  sucks  counted  during  8  minutes  of  non-nutritive 
sucking;  (2)  total  crying,  the  number  of  cries  during  60  seconds  following 
removal  of  nipple;  and  (3)  reaction  time  to  nipple  removal.  The  higher  the 
score,  according  to  the  theory,  the  more  lethargic  and  ineffectual  as  a 
feeder  was  the  infant. 

A  significant  positive  correlation  appeared  between  newborn  lethargy 
and  preschool  contact  seeking.  The  higher  the  lethargy  score,  the  gicatcr 
the  amount  of  contact  seeking.  Another  correlation  tended  to  substantiate 
these  findings.  The  higher  the  FSD  index,  the  less  was  the  child's  ability  to 
defend  himself  against  the  aggression  of  peers.  These  results  we  it* 
indicative  of  a  distinct  newborn  behavior  pattern  in  children  born  into 
Jarge,  dense  families.  The  pattern  is  characterized  as  lethargy  and  is 
predictive  of  heightened  contact-seeking  behavior  at  the  age  of  2Vz  years. 
By  looking  at  the  lesults  of  the  two  FSD  studies  together,  it  is  possible 
to  say  that  children  born  into  large,  dense  families  will  tend  at  the 
preschool  age  to  be  dependent  in  this  one  sense.  One  component  of  the 
tendency  is  biological,  a  congenital  pattern  of  lethargy,  and  the  other  may 
be  social,  low-maternal  availability. 

In  one  way  these  findings  simply  take  a  place  in  the  large  body  of 
evidence  detailing  the  risk  of  deleterious  effects  on  children  born  into 
families  with  many  siblings  closely  spaced.  Other  lesearchers  have  shown, 
for  example,  that  lates  of  prematurity,  maldevelopment,  and  mental 
deficiency  increase  with  an  increase  in  parity,  especially  after  the  third 
and  fourth  child.  Infant  mortality  was  found  to  be  highest  when  the  birth 
interval  was  less  than  2  yeais;  poor  physical  status  and  low  IQ  scores  have 
been  associated  with  an  increase  in  family  size,  One  epidemiological  study 
discovered  a  strong  correlation  between  paranoia  and  the  presence  of  a 
sibling  less  than  2  years  older. 

In  another  way,  however,  the  findings  add  a  critical  new  idea  to  the 
study  of  family  effects  on  child  development.  Most  investigators  have 

230 


assumed  that  the  controlling  factor  was  social  class  because  large  family 
size  has  been  associated  repeatedly  with  low  socioeconomic  status.  But 
Mis.  Waldiop  found  no  relation  in  her  sample  between  FSD  and  a  widely 
used  index  of  the  father's  occupational-educational  level.  The  results 
indicate,  then,  that  families  on  the  higher  socioeconomic  levels,  if  they  are 
large  and  dense,  are  not  exempt  from  the  consequence  of  later  children 
being  more  lethargic  and  less  effectual  feeders  than  their  siblings.  It  might 
be  well  to  note  explicitly  that  these  findings  apply  to  large  numbers  of 
subjects,  but  not  necessarily  to  an  individual  child  in  any  one  family. 

The  immediate  value  of  this  part  of  the  program  lies  in  the  success  of  its 
unique  methodology.  The  investigators  demonstrated  how  to  relate 
piospective  and  restrospective  data.  Their  experience  leads  them  to  believe 
that  mediator  variables  in  addition  to  maternal  leport  and  FSD— for 
instance,  social  class-might  be  used  to  link  newborn  and  preschool 
behavior  in  longitudinal  and  cross-sectional  samples. 

Dr.  Bell  and  his  colleagues  plan  to  complete  the  data  analyses  of  the 
newborn-preschool  measures  already  in  hand,  and  to  make  behavioral 
measures  of  infants  born  to  couples  studied  in  early  marriage  by  other 
investigators  of  the  Child  Research  Branch.  They  are  also  raising  new 
questions  about  early  social  development.  Does  social  responsiveness  vaiy 
in  infants  as  young  as  3  or  4  months?  What  are  the  effects  on  mothers  of 
specific  newborn  characteristics?  In  particular,  how  docs  a  restless,  hard- 
to-quiet  infant  affect  the  patterning  and  quality  of  his  mother's  care  and 
peculiarly  shape  the  initial  opportunities  of  learning?  Are  there  any 
characteristics  of  sleep  during  the  first  months  of  life  associated  with  the 
congenital  behavior  syndrome?  Having  located  some  beginning  and  end 
points,  the  researchers  now  would  like  to  trace  processes  involved  in  the 
transition. 

The  observations  and  ratings  made  in  the  nursery  school  also  weie 
employed  to  test  another  hypothesis,  one  relating  social  behavior  to  later 
cognitive  development.  Although  this  research  question  was  not  part  of 
the  original  program  goal,  the  necessary  data  were  there  so  the  investiga- 
tors took  the  opportunity  to  use  them  in  a  new  departure. 

Social  behavior  and  cognitive  sfy/e.-Eailier  studies  of  cognitive  style 
had  found  that  a  maternal  attitude  fostering  independence  and  interaction 
with  the  environment  was  associated  with  nonverbal  skills;  maternal 
fostering  of  dependence  tended  to  develop  a  child's  verbal  abilities.  A 
child*s  style  of  dependency  has  been  related  to  another  aspect  of 
cognition,  the  way  disparate  objects  are  described  and  grouped.  Two  of 
investigators.  Dr.  Frank  A.  Pedersen  and  Paul  H.  Wender  (the  latter  is 
working  currently  in  the  NIMH  Laboratory  of  Clinical  Psychology) 
wanted  to  see  if  the  social  behavior  measures  of  preschool  children  were 
predictive  of  the  children's  cognitive  functioning  at  age  6. 

The  investigators  gave  tests  of  cognition  to  a  group  of  6-year-old  boys, 
graduates  of  the  first  nursery  school  sample.  One  test  was  the  Wechsler 
Intelligence  Scale  for  Children,  which  measures  verbal  and  nonverbal 
abilities.  Another  was  a  new  sorting  test  designed  to  elicit  a  child's  style  of 
categorizing.  (It  was  devised  by  Dr.  Irving  Sigel,  of  the  Merrill-Palmer 


Institute.)  The  third  was  the  Cluldien's  Embedded  Figures  Test,  wlw 
measures  the  ability  to  extract  a  relevant  item  from  an  irrelevant  sii 
rounding  field.  It  tests  peiceptual  aspects  of  cognition  and  is  thought  1 
index  an  individual's  dependence  on  context  in  acts  of  perception  an 
judgment.  In  a  seiies  of  studies  that  has  extended  over  several  years,  I) 
Herman  A.  Witkm,  of  the  State  Umveisity  of  New  York,  has  shown  th: 
those  who  are  good  at  picking  out  the  figure  embedded  in  the  surroundin 
field  (field  independent)  tend  to  be  socially  autonomous  and  to  take  n 
active,  analytical  view  of  their  woild.  Field-dependent  subjects,  coi 
versely,  are  likely  to  be  socially  dependent  and  to  see  their  world  i 
diffuse,  global  terms. 

Each  boy's  peiformancc  on  these  tasks  was  compared  with  his  ratinp 
on  four  different  kinds  of  behavior  reflecting  dependence  or  autonomy 
measured  when  he  attended  the  nursery  school;  Contact  seeking,  al 
tention  seeking,  orality  (thumb  sucking,  mouthing),  and  sustained 
directed  activity.  These  were  the  findings: 

•  More    contact-oriented,    attention-seeking   boys   in   the   prcschoo 
period  showed  lower  nonverbal  IQ's  at  age  6  years  than  did  boys  no 
high  on   those  preschool  ratings.  Those  who  had  high  ratings  or 
sustained,  directed  activity  in  the  nursery  school  were  high  on  ncm 
verbal  IQ's  at  the  later  age. 

•  Boys  lated  high  on  contact  seeking  and  orality  in  the  earlier  study 
were,   at  6   years  of  age,  high  on  the  use  of  the  categorization 
response  that  reflects  the  global  approach. 

•  Those  high  on  sustained,  directed  activity  in  the  nursery  school  wore 
more  likely  than  the  others  to  use  a  superordinate  concept  in  their 
categorizations  (recognizing,  for  example,  that  a  saw  and  a  pair  of 
pliers  were  both  tools),  a  style  the  investigators  suggest  is  a  high 
ojder  of  abstraction  for  6-yeai-olds. 

Results  of  the  Children's  Embedded  Figures  Test  correlated  significant- 
ly only  with  the  early  measures  of  orality,  but  all  associations  were  in  (he 
predicted  direction. 

The  investigators  concluded  that  preschool  dependency  behavior  is 
indeed  predictive  of  certain  kinds  of  cognitive  functioning  at  age  6  years. 
Theirs  is  the  first  leport  of  these  relationships  at  so  early  an  age. 

On  the  basis  of  the  data  available  to  them,  Drs.  Pederscn  and  Wonder 
weie  not  prepared  to  say  just  what  social  or  biological  factois  might  be 
involved  in  the  relation  between  social  behavior  and  cognitive  style.  They 
were  able,  however,  to  rule  out  the  influence  of  socioeconomic  class.  In 
their  sample,  family  socioeconomic  levels  were  not  decisive  relative  to  the 
differences  in  cognition  displayed  by  the  boys,  Perhaps  maternal  or 
paternal  attitudes  are  primarily  mediating  factois,  as  earlier  studies 
indicate;  or  perhaps,  as  the  investigators  suggest,  congenital  factois  make 
important  contributions. 

They  made  this  suggestion  after  carrying  the  study  one  step  further. 
Dr.  Wender  took  a  close  look  at  the  family  reports  of  four  boys  who  were 
extreme  on  social  and  cognitive  measures,  hoping  to  turn  up  some 
nUusible  leads  on  the  parental  characteristics  differentiating  the  boys' 


polar  positions.  Not  only  did  definite  patterns  fail  to  emerge,  on  the 
contiary,  the  boys  came  from  remaikably  diverse  backgrounds.  The 
investigators  know  that  their  review  of  the  families  is  not  reason  to 
discount  the  influence  of  parental  characteristics  on  the  social-cognitive 
developmental  axis.  For  one  thing,  their  family  information  was  based 
largely  on  contacts  with  mothers  and  did  not  include  data  for  an  analysis 
of  paternal  effects.  For  another,  current  learning  theory  suggests  the  im- 
portance of  specific  patterns  and  timing  of  rewards;  this  area  of  the  chil- 
dren's background  was  unknown  to  the  investigatois.  Still,  the  failure  of 
the  data  to  yield  clues  implicating  parental  characteristics  calls  into 
question  hypotheses  about  the  effect  on  cognition  of  maternal  vaiiables 
such  as  affection  and  neglect.  An  alternate  hypothesis  worth  considering, 
the  investigators  say,  is  that  "congenital  differences  in  children  generate 
the  relationships  between  social  behavior  and  cognitive  functioning." 


Intramural:  NIMH 

Date  of  Interviews    Mar.  24,  1966 

Refeiences 

Accelciated  Longitudinal  Studies 
Bell,    R    Q    An  experimental   test  of  the  accelerated  longitudinal  approach.  Child 

Developm.,  1954,25,281-286. 
Bell,   R    Q.  Retrospective  and  prospective  views  of  early  personality  development. 

Men  ill-Palmer  Quart  Behav.  Developm.,  1960,6,131-144. 

Effects  of  Children  on  Parents 

Bell,  R.  Q.  The  effect  on  the  family  of  a  limitation  in  coping  ability  in  the  child:  a 
research  approach  and  a  finding.  Merrill-Palme}  Quart,  Behav.  Developm.,  1 964,  1 0, 
129-142. 

Bell,  R.  Q.  The  problem  of  direction  of  effects  in  studies  of  parents  and  children. 
Paper  presented  at  the  Conference  on  Research  Methodology  in  Parent-Child  Inter- 
action, held  under  auspices  of  the  Department  of  Pediatrics,  Upstate  Medical 
Center,  State  University  of  New  York,  Syracuse,  N.Y.,  October  1964. 

Newborns 

Bell,  R.  Q.  Some  factors  to  be  controlled  in  studies  of  the  behavior  of  newborns. 
Biologta Neonatorum,  1963,  5,  200-214. 

Bell,  R.  Q.  Level  of  arousal  in  breast-fed  and  bottle-fed  human  newborns.  Psychosom, 
Med.,  1966,28,  177-180. 

Bell,  R.  Q.,  &  Costello,  N.  S.  Three  tests  for  sex  differences  in  tactile  sensitivity  in  the 
newborn.  Biologia  Neonatomm,  1964,7,  335-347. 

Bell,  R.  Q.,  &  Darling,  J.  F.  The  prone  head  reaction  in  the  human  neonate;  relation 
with  sex  and  tactile  sensitivity.  Child  Developm. ,  1965,36,943-949. 

Weller,  G.  M.,  &  Bell,  R.  Q.  Basal  skin  conductance  and  neonatal  state.  Child  De- 
velopm., 1965,36,647-657. 

Relation  of  Newborn  and  Preschool  Behavior 

Waldrop,  M.  F  ,  &  Bell,  R.  Q.  Relation  of  preschool  dependency  behavior  to  family 
size  and  density.  Child  Developm, ,  1964,35,  1187-1195. 

Waldrop,  M.  F.,  &  Bell,  R.  Q,  Effects  of  family  size  and  density  on  newborn  charac- 
teristics. A mer  J.  Orthopsyclriat,,  1966,36,544-550, 

233 


Social  Behavior  and  Cognitive  Functioning 

Pedersen,  F.  A.,  &  Wender,  P  H.  Early  social  correlates  of  cognitive  functioning  i 
young  children,  Paper  presented  at  the  Society  for  Research  in  Child  Developmeii 
Minneapolis,  March  1965. 


234 


Investigator 

Fred  L.  Strodtbeck,  Ph.  D. 

University  of  Chicago 

Chicago,  III. 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 

In  the  give  and  take  of  family  discussions  an  investigator  supported  by 
the  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health  is  seeking  information  on  factors 
that  influence  the  development  of  a  young  person's  intelligence  and 
personality. 

Dr.  Fred  L.  Strodtbeck  and  his  research  group  at  the  University  of 
Chicago  use  what  they  call  the  technique  of  revealed  differences  to  study 
how  meinbeis  of  a  family  interact.  The  father,  the  mother,  and  one  or 
more  teenage  children  are  given  copies  of  a  set  of  questions  and  asked  to 
respond  individually.  Some  examples: 

Should  the  parents  of  a  13-year-old  girl  have  any  say  about  who  is 
invited  to  a  party  at  the  girl's  house,  or  should  the  decision  be 
entirely  up  to  the  girl? 

A  high  school  junior  likes  math  and  would  like  to  take  a  geometry 
course  but  thinks  she  won't  do  well.  Should  she  take  the  course,  or 
should  she  take  a  course  that  she  would  be  more  certain  of  doing 
well  in? 

Having  a  nice  personality  and  being  well  liked  are  more  essential 
for  success  than  any  particular  type  of  skill.  Do  you  agree? 
The    families   are    then   asked   to   talk   over   those   problems— always 
numerous—on  which  differences  of  opinion  have  been  revealed  and  to  try 
to  agree  on  an  answer.  At  this  point  the  experimenter  leaves  the  room,  but 
the  discussions  are  recorded  and  later  analyzed.  Some  of  the  cases  on 
which  there  was  disagreement  are  discussed  by  three  or  more  members  of 
the  family,  some  by  only  two. 

Members  of  the  family,  the  grantee  explains,  have  sized  up  one  another 
over  the  years  and  now  have  certain  expectations  of  how  each  will  behave. 
Because  of  these  expectations,  a  family  discussion  is  likely  to  bring  out 
typical  behavior.  Consequently,  the  discussion,  or  interaction,  is  analogous 
to  a  sample  of  blood:  Analysis  provides  clues  in  the  one  case  as  to  how  the 
body  is  functioning,  in  the  other,  the  family. 

The  grantee  believes  that  the  study  of  family  interactions  will  get  at 
some  of  the  basic  reasons  for  the  ways,  healthy  or  unhealthy,  that 
children  adjust  to  life  and  go  on  to  influence  the  adjustment  of  others,  in 
particular  their  own  children.  Findings  will  be  checked,  it  is  planned, 
when  the  children  in  the  families  studied  have  become  adults, 

235 


The  judges  who  listen  to  the  tape  recordings  of  the  discussions  give  each 
participant  a  power  score.  On  a  given  question,  for  example,  if  only  the 
father  gave  a  certain  response  but  then,  during  the  discussion,  won  the 
others  to  his  way  of  thinking,  he  gets  four  points  and  the  others  none,  If, 
instead,  the  mother  and  child  won  the  father  over  to  their  side,  they  each 
get  two  points  and  the  fathei  none. 

The  judges  also  score  the  participants  on  such  matters  as  the  conviction 
with  which  a  position  was  held;  the  contribution  made  by  each  pctson 
toward  (a)  reaching  the  decision,  and  (b)  preventing  and  relieving  strain 
during  the  discussion;  self-assurance;  and  the  warmth  and  understanding 
shown  to  each  of  the  other  persons. 

Among  other  points  considered  during  the  analysis: 

•  The  family's  health,  or  the  quality  of  family  relationships,  as  infencd 
from  how  well  the  family  went  about  reaching  consensus  on  a  given 
problem. 

•  The  family's  success  in  dispelling  any  tension  so  that  members  were 
free  to  move  on  to  the  next  question  with  a  clean  slate. 

•  The  masculinity  of  the  father  and  the  femininity  of  the  mother. 


High  I Q  vs.  Average  I Q  Teenagers 

In  a  study  neanng  completion,  the  University  of  Chicago  group  has 
used  the  revealed-difference  technique  to  study  families  having  an  adoles- 
cent child  between  13  and  15  with  very  high  intellectual  ability.  Five  of 
the  children  were  girls>  five  were  boys.  They  had  IQ's  higher  than  1 60. 

The  controls  weie  classmates  of  average  ability.  Their  IQ's  ranged  from 
110  to  120.  Their  fathers  had  been  matched  by  religion  and  socio- 
economic  status  with  the  fathers  of  the  high  IQ  group. 

One  of  Dr,  Strodtbeck's  giaduate  students  had  theorized,  in  suggesting 
the  study,  that  the  children  with  extremely  good  intellects  would  tend  to 
be  more  isolated  and  withdrawn  than  the  others  and  would  thus  make  for 
poorer  family  relations.  But  the  opposite  has  proved  true:  Family  health 
was  considerably  better  and  the  relations  much  warmer  in  the  families 
with  a  brilliant  child.  This  was  not  owing  solely  to  the  direct  contribution 
of  the  brilliant  child,  for  when  the  parents  were  discussing  a  problem 
alone,  their  warmth  and  helpfulness  were  even  greater  than  when  the  child 
was  present.  In  the  other  families,  when  the  father  and  mother  were  alone, 
their  relations  giew  rougher. 

The  high  IQ  family,  Dr.  Strodtbeck  reports,  has  much  greater  problem- 
solving  abilities.  When  the  parents  and  the  teenager  talk  with  one  another, 
they  show  greater  precision  in  identifying  their  differences,  and  greater 
clarity  in  reconciling  them.  Presumably  this  is  explained  in  part  by  the 
high  intelligence  of  the  youngster  and  perhaps  of  his  parents  as  well.  At 
the  same  time,  Dr.  Strodtbeck  believes  that  growth  of  intellectual 
competence  is  fostered  by  the  kind  of  warm  family  relations  he  finds 
displayed  by  the  high  IQ  families  in  this  study. 

236 


"We  think  that  if  a  person's  high  intellectual  capacity  is  to  be  realized," 
the  grantee  says,  "he  will  have  to  be  relatively  free  of  internal  concerns,  so 
that  he  can  continue  to  take  in  information  about  the  world  and  to 
stimulate  other  people  to  talk  in  ways  that  enable  him  to  learn  from  them. 
If  a  child's  family  life  fulfills  his  needs,  his  energy  is  released  for  learning 
more  about  the  outside  world.  But  if  he  is  tied  up  in  neurotic  conflicts  in 
his  family,  he  will  not  have  the  zest  for  taking  in  information  from 
outside." 

Recent  studies  have  shown,  the  grantee  notes,  that  the  IQ's  of  children 
hospitalized  as  schizophrenics  had  started  sliding  down  before  the  trouble 
became  apparent.  "When  you're  working  on  an  extremely  tough 
problem,"  he  explains,  "you  just  don't  have  as  much  energy  for  encoding 
information  from  the  world  around  you." 

Using  the  revealed-difference  technique,  another  investigator  has 
compared  parents  of  children  hospitalized  for  schizophrenia  with  parents 
of  children  hospitalized  for  tuberculosis.  He  found,  Dr.  Strodtbeck 
reports,  that  the  husbands  and  wives  of  the  schizophrenic  children  failed 
to  reconcile  their  views-after  originally  giving  different  answers-five  or 
six  times  as  often  as  the  parents  of  tubercular  children.  Dr.  Strodtbeck 
believes  that  studies  underway  or  planned  by  his  research  team  will  help 
uncover  some  of  the  elements  in  family  relations  responsible  for  such 
differences. 

In  addition  to  the  teenagers  and  their  parents  in  the  1Q  study,  Dr. 
Strodtbeck's  long-term  research  program  embraces  half  a  dozen  groups, 
among  them: 

1 .  A  number  of  young  men  who,  with  their  parents,  were  first  studied 
by  Dr.  Strodtbeck  in  1952,  in  New  Haven,  when  they  were  from  13  to  15 
years  old.  The  fiist  study  showed  that  the  higher  a  father's  power  score, 
the  lower  the  achievement  values  expressed  by  his  son.  The  followup 
study  finds  that  this  effect  apparently  has  a  lasting  influence.  Young  men 
whose  fathers  scored  high  in  power  tend  to  have  chosen,  or  drifted  into, 
occupations  lower  than  their  fathers'.  On  the  other  hand,  the  sons  of 
fathers  who  were  low  in  power  tend  to  have  outpaced  them.  Though  other 
findings  await  analysis  of  the  data,  the  grantee  believes  that  one  of  them 
will  verify  and  extend  a  finding  of  the  IQ  study.  The  New  Haven  study 
will  show,  he  believes,  that  warmth  in  family  interactions  relates  not  only 
to  functioning  at  higher  IQ  levels  in  early  adolescence  but  also  to  higher 
accomplishment  as  an  adult. 

2.  A  group  of  "pathological  families"— families  with  children  in  their 
teens  who  had  needed  treatment  for  a  behavior  problem  from  3  to  1 0 
years  before  the  study. 

3.  A  number  of  two-child  families  in  which  the  parents  are  relatively 
young,  about  40  years  old  when  their  first  child  is  a  high  school  junior, 
and  a  number  in  which  the  parents  are   10  years  older.  This  study  is 
underway.  Among  the  findings  to  date:  The  older  parents  permit  their 
children  to  exert  more  influence  than  the  younger,  as  measured  by  the 
power  scores  achieved  during  the  family  discussions. 

237 


The  Acquirement  of  Masculinity  and  Femininity 

The  investigators  are  particularly  interested  in  the  processes  by  whi 
child  takes  on  masculine  or  feminine  attitudes,  since  the  way  a  pe: 
identifies  his  sex  role  can  be  an  important  factor  in  his  emotional  he 
throughout  life. 

Dr.  Strodtbeck  explains  that  masculinity  is  taken  to  imply  I 
headedness  under  pressure,  the  ability  to  strike  out  effectively  win 
crisis  is  at  hand,  an  unwillingness  to  be  involved  in  trivial  concerns, 
ability  to  act  considerately  and  helpfully  without  the  fear  of  being  • 
sidered  effeminate,  and  curiosity  about  what's  going  on. 

On  the  other  hand,  femininity  is  taken  to  include  a  greater  inleiu 
persons  than  in  things,  concern  for  the  quality  of  the  relations  betv 
persons,  the  management  of  a  paired  rclationship-as  that  betu 
husband  and  wife-so  that  the  members  are  complementary  nHhurl 
competitive,  patience  in  matters  involving  the  welfare  of  others, 
concern  and  support  for  members  of  one's  group  in  time  of  crisis. 

Among  the  families  in  the  high  versus  low  IQ  study,  the  grantee  rept 
brilliant  children  displayed  to  a  relatively  high  degree  characteristics  of 
role  commonly  ascribed  to  the  opposite  sex.  That  is,  a  brilliant  boy 
judged  to  be  more  like  his  mother  than  an  average  boy,  but  at  (he  s 
time  not  any  the  less  like  his  father.  A  girl  with  a  high  IQ  was  judged  l< 
more  like  her  father  than  an  average  girl,  but  at  the  same  time  nol  any 
less  like  her  mother. 

"We  believe,"  says  Dr.  Strodtbeck,  "that  the  greater  warmth  of  the  1 
IQ  families  makes  it  easier  for  a  child  to  identify  with  the  parent  of 
opposite  sex  and  to  do  so  without  becoming  anxious.  Thus  a  child 
behave  more  flexibly." 

As  evidence   for  such   identification,   the  study  has  determined 
correlation  between  the  power  scores  of  each  of  the  parents  and  the  lo 
rating  the  child  has  given  to  himself.  The  parent  whoso  power  is  corrcl; 
most  positively  with  the  child's  own  sense  of  potency,  the  study  ussui 
will  be  the  parent  with  whom  the  child  most  closely  identifies. 

The  findings; 

•  Boys  with  average  IQ-siighl  positive  correlation  with  father's  pin 
negative  with  mother's. 

•  Boys   with    high   IQ-positivo  correlation   with    mother's  pin 
negative  with  father's. 

•  Gills   with    avciagc   IQ-slighl    positive   correlation   with    moth 
power,  negative  with  father's. 

•  Girls  with  high  IQ-positive  correlation  with  father's  power,  ncgu 
'lh  mother's. 

1J   Oirodlbeck  emphasizes  that  these  findings  are  based  on  a  stud] 
~ses.  But  they  make  sense,  he  says,  if  one  of  the  component 
*encc  is  "freedom  from  phobic  avoidance  of  demon  Is  eha 
ipposite  sex  culture." 

is  investigator  sees  it,  the  development  of  high  inlclligi! 
ind  at  the  same  time  contributes  to,  family  warmth 


understanding.  In  such  an  atmosphere  the  gifted  child  is  freer  both  to 
develop  his  capabilities  and  to  adopt  a  broader  set  of  worthwhile  attitudes 
and  qualities  even  though  some  of  these  are  generally  considered  charac- 
teristic of  the  opposite  sex. 

Research  Grant:  MH  5572 


239 


Investigator. 

Daniel  Offer,  M.D. 

Michaei  Reese  Hospital 
Chicago,  III, 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Psychiatrists  try  to  help  sick  people  become  well,  but  what  is  welt 
What  do  we  mean  by  normal  mental  Health? 

Psychiatry  has  too  long  neglected  this  question,  says  Dr.  Roy  R 
Grinker,  Sr.,  an  eminent  psychiatrist  and  director  of  the  Institute  fo 
Psychosomatic  and  Psychiatric  Research  and  Training,  Michael  Recsi 
Hospital  and  Medical  Center,  Chicago.  Hence,  as  one  part  of  a  broac 
lesearch  program,  membeis  of  the  Institute's  staff  have  been  trying  to  fim 
some  of  the  answers.  Their  work  is  supported  by  the  National  Institute  ol 
Mental  Health, 

This  report  presents  highlights  of  three  studies:  First,  a  continuing 
investigation  of  normal  high  school  students;  the  work  of  a  team  headed 
by  Dr.  Daniel  Offer,  the  institute's  assistant  director;  second,  a  study, 
nearing  completion,  by  Dr.  David  Marcus  and  several  associates,  including 
Dr,  Offer,  of  what  seems  to  be  an  important  difference  between  families 
that  do  not  have  a  disturbed  adolescent  child  and  families  that  do;  third,  a 
completed  study  by  Dr,  Griuker  of  a  group  of  normal  colleges  students. 


f.  Psychiatry  Views  the  Normal  Adolescent 

After  some  years  of  daily  contact  with  adolescents  who  were  disturbed 
or  delinquent,  or  both,  Dr.  Offer  decided  to  take  a  good  look  at  the  other 
kind,  the  normal  ones-their  backgrounds,  personalities*  viewpoints, 
worries,  and  behavior. 

In  adolescents,  he  points  out,  it  has  been  unusually  difficult  to  dis- 
tinguish health  from  illness,  normal  turmoil  from  pathological  process. 
Even  in  the  case  of  psychotherapists,  ideas  about  the  characteristics 
making  for  normality  in  teenagers  have  been  generally  based  on  memories 
of  a  person's  own  adolescence  and  observations  of  his  own  children.  With 
the  understanding  to  be  gained  by  a  systematic  study,  Dr.  Offer  reasoned, 
psychotherapists  would  have  a  more  valid  baseline  for  judging  disturbed 
teenagers,  and  families,  schools  and  society  in  general  would  be  better 
able  to  handle  problems  presented  by  teenagers  and  to  recognize,  prevent, 
™A  ""*"*  ""*u  abnormal  behavior. 


Dr.  Offer  is  now  halfway  through  an  intensive  3-year  study  of  84 
normal  adolescents— boys  from  middle-class  families  in  the  Chicago 
metropolitan  area  selected  fiom  two  public  high  schools  during  their 
freshman  year. 

Among  the  findings  to  date; 

1 .  The  normal  adolescent,  like  the  disturbed  one,  has  feelings  of  shame, 
guilt,  depression,  or  anxiety.  But  he  is  less  afraid  to  look  at  himself  and  to 
admit  his   feelings.  Psychiatrists  would  be  happy,  Dr.  Offer  remarks,  if 
patients  even  at  the  end  of  therapy  were  as  aware  of  their  problems  as 
these  normal  boys  are  of  theirs. 

2.  Many  of  these  atomic-age  boys  are  worried  about  the  same  things 
that  boys  have  worried  about  for  generations— including  sex,  religion,  and 
money.  Only  a  few  are  worried  about  the  state  of  the  world.  A  teenager's 
three  most  difficult  problems,  these  boys  say,  are  to  do  as  well  educa- 
tionally and  vocationally  as  his  family  expects,  to  contiol  his  impulses, 
and  to  get  along  with  other  people. 

3.  The    boys    have    a    conservative   sexual    code    and,   through    the 
sophomore  year  at  least,  they  behave  conservatively.  Many  of  them  day- 
dream about  a  specific  girl  but  do  not  readily  admit  it.  Five  percent  go 
steady;  35  percent  group-date  only;  35  percent  do  not  date  at  all.  Typi- 
cally the  boys  are  concerned  with  how  to  behave  when  out  with  a  girl. 
Most  are  interested  in  sports— as  a  means  of  displaying  masculinity  and  of 
sublimating,  Dr.  Offer  believes,  both  aggressive  and  sexual  impulses. 

4.  About  20  percent  smoke.  A  few  diink. 

5.  Many  of  these  normal  boys— generally  when  they  were  only  12  or 
13— have  performed  minor  delinquent  acts  and  have  associated  with  delin- 
quents. Twenty-five  peicent  of  them,  in  fact,  have  been  involved  with  the 
police  over  such  incidents  as  stealing  from  a  drugstore,  fighting,  throwing 
bottles  on  highways,  or  overtmning  garbage  cans.  After  a  boy  has  been  in 
trouble  once  or  twice,  though,  he  seems  to  have  learned  his  lesson,  He 
docs  not  make  delinquency  a  pattern.  Nevertheless  he  sympathizes  with 
juvenile  delinquents  and  ascribes  their  troubles  entirely  to  their  parents 
and  to  society. 

6.  These  normal  teenagers  are  not  inclined  to  rebel  against  either  then 
parents  or  their  parents'  generation.  They  see  clearly  what  values  their 
parents  hold,  and  they  tend  to  hold  the  same  ones  themselves.  When 
adolescents  and  parents  disagree,  it  is  on  such  matters  as  the  use  of  the  car 
and  the  time  to  come  home  at  night.  The  boys  find  their  fathers  reliable, 
their  mothers  understanding.  They  feel  closer  to  their  mothers  and  can 
more  easily  discuss  emotional  problems  with  them. 

7.  Members  of  the  group  express  definite  ideas  about  what  they  are 
going  to  do  when  they  have  finished  school.  But  they  tend  to  change  these 
ideas  as  time  goes  by. 

For  the  most  part  these  findings  stem  from  four  40-minute  psychiatric 
interviews  with  each  boy,  spread  over  a  period  of  a  year  and  a  half.  Four 
more  such  interviews  are  planned,  as  are  less  intensive  interviews  with  the 
parents.  The  complete  description  of  these  normal  adolescents  will  also 
take  into  account  teacher  ratings,  school  performance  records,  and  the 

241 


results  of  projective  testing  as  other  means  of  getting  information  a  bo 
hopes,  fears,  and  anxieties. 

How  does  one  pick  a  normal  adolescent?  Dr.  Offer  and  his  associat 
did  it  by  going  to  two  high  schools  m  the  fall  of  1 962,  one  in  a  suburb  ju 
north  of  Chicago  and  the  other  in  a  suburb  just  south,  and  giving  sever 
hundred  fieshmen  boys  two  tests.  One  was  the  Self-image  Questionnaii 
for  Adolescent  Boys,  developed  by  Dr.  Offer  and  an  associate,  Dr.  Mclvi 
Sabshin,  now  head  of  the  Department  of  Psychiatry  of  the  University  c 
Illinois   College  of  Medicine;  the  other,  the  Bell  Adjustment  Inventor; 
Each   comprised   a   number  of  scales,  or  subtests,  and  each  scale  \va 
intended  to  measure  one  aspect  of  how  the  boy  regarded  himself  or  hi 
world.  Taken  all  together,  the  giantee  believes,  they  provided  a  goo. 
picture  of  a  boy's  ability-in  terms  of  his  emotional  adjustment-to  mee 
his  problems, 

Of  the  boys  tested,  Dr.  Offer  selected  the  114-about  a  third  of  (In 
total— whose  scores  on  all  the  subtests  had  been  closest  to  the  average 
Thus  he  eliminated  the  boys  who  scored  highest  (and  were,  presumably 
extremely  well  adjusted)  and  those  who  scored  lowest.  He  also  eliminatec 
those  who  scored  much  higher  than  average  in  some  areas  and  much  lowei 
than  average  in  others.  So  in  this  study  the  normal  adolescent  is  what  Dr. 
Offer  calls  the  modal,  or  average  adolescent. 

After  making  his  selections,  the  investigator  checked  with  the  school 
authorities  and  found  that  3  of  the  boys  were  serious  behavior  problems 
(in  a  randomly  chosen  group  of  the  same  size,  15  would  have  been).  These 
were  dropped  from  the  study.  A  few  boys  declined  to  participate  and  a 
number  moved. 

Dr.  Offer  emphasizes  that  most  of  the  boys  being  studied  come  from 
families  at  one  or  another  level  of  the  middle  class.  About  10  percent 
come  from  stable,  working-class  families,  as  would  be  expected  ftom  the 
proportion  of  such  families  in  the  population  represented,  and  about  7 
percent  are  Negroes,  as  again  would  be  expected.  Presumably  the  study's 
findings  would  hold  true  for  middle-class  communities  in  any  metro- 
politan area. 

None  of  these  well-adjusted  boys  has  lost  a  parent  because  of  divorce, 
only  three  because  of  separation,  only  two  because  of  death;  in  sum,  only 
6  percent  come  from  disrupted  homes.  This  compares  with  9  percent  of 
the  general  population  of  teenagers  (aged  18)  in  the  two  communities 
studied  and  with  45  percent  of  the  delinquent  adolescents  studied  by 
another  investigatoi. 

More  than  half  of  the  selected  group  are  either  the  oldest  or  the  only 
child  in  the  family— a  striking  statistic,  says  Dr.  Offer,  but  one  that  may 
only  reflect  the  tendency  of  families  to  move  to  the  suburbs  when  the 
first  child  is  ready  for  school. 

It  developed  also  that  the  group  had  its  share  of  honor  students-12 
per  cent —during  the  freshman  year,  but  no  failures. 

When  Dr.  Offer  was  organizing  his  project,  some  of  his  associates 
doubted  that  it  could  succeed.  Normal  teenagers,  they  argued,  would 
never  show  up  for  appointments  with  a  psychiatrist -they'd  feel  disgraced. 

242 


As  a  matter  of  fact,  more  than  half  of  the  group  did  rniss  theii  appoint- 
ments during  the  early  months  and  had  to  be  scheduled  again.  This 
proportion  has  now  dropped  to  one-fifth,  which  the  investigatoi  consideis 
good,  because  the  interviews  are  held  after  school  hours  and  frequently 
involve  two  round  trips  for  the  parent  who  does  the  driving. 

Teenagers  are  extremely  egotistical,  Dr.  Offer  notes.  They  participate  in 
the  project  because  someone  is  interested  in  learning  what  they -and  not 
adults-think  about  teenageis  and  the  world.  Teenagers  are  also  extremely 
altruistic.  They  participate  because  they  have  been  told  that  in  the  long 
run  the  project  will  help  other  teenagers. 

What  does  he  think  of  these  normal  youngsteis?  "Oh  my!"  the  psy- 
chiatrist  says,  "They're  tremendous!" 


IJ.  Teen-Agers  in  Trouble:  A  Communications  Breakdown? 

Parents  requesting  psychiatric  help  for  an  adolescent  child,  psychiatrists 
at  Michael  Reese  observed  over  a  3-year  period,  complain  most  frequently 
about' 

1.  Delinquent   behavior,    ranging    from   assault   and   major   theft   to 
isolated  incidences  of  vandalism  in  school. 

2.  Difficulty  in  making  and  keeping  friends. 

3.  Inability  to  adjust  to  school  situations. 

4.  Inability  on  the  part  of  the  parents  to  "manage"  the  adolescent. 
The  adolescents,  on  the  other  hand,  most  frequently  complain  that  their 

parents  do  not  understand  them;  hence  their  problems.  Their  second  most 
frequent  complaint  is  that  they  do  not  understand  their  parents. 

The  investigators  asked  themselves:  In  a  family  where  the  teenagers  are 
apparently  normal,  have  the  parents  and  youngsters  been  understanding 
each  other  better  than  in  a  family  where  a  teenager  is  disturbed? 

Twenty  middle-class  families  were  studied,  each  intact  and  each 
including  at  least  two  adolescents  ranging  in  age  from  14  to  19  and  in 
good  physical  health.  Half  the  families  were  classified  as  normal,  or  non- 
disturbed:  no  member  had,  or  had  had,  any  obvious  emotional  trouble. 
The  other  half  were  classifed  as  disturbed:  one  of  the  adolescents  had 
been  hospitalized  for  psychiatric  reasons,  the  diagnosis  in  five  cases  was 
schizophrenia  and  in  five  cases  character  disorders  that  had  led  to  such 
actions  as  car  stealing,  assaulting  parents,  and,  in  the  case  of  one  girl, 
becoming  pregnant. 

Then  the  Q-sort  technique  was  used  to  obtain  from  each  adolescent 
both  a  description  of  himself  and  an  account  of  how  he  thought  his 
mother  would  describe  him.  Similarly,  each  mother  described  her  son 
both  as  she  would  have  liked  him  to  be  and  as  the  thought  he  would 
describe  himself. 

Analysis  of  the  data  leads  to  these  principal  findings: 

1 .  In  normal  families,  mothers  and  children  were  in  good  communica- 
tion. An  adolescent  understood  his  mother's  expectations  for  him  and  was 
able  in  turn  to  convey  his  own  viewpoints  to  his  mother. 

243 


2.  In  disturbed  families,  the  patient  and  the  mother  were  in  poor  ecu 
munication.  The  lines  between  the  other  disturbed  adolescent  and  tl 
mother  were  open  but  not  to  the  same  extent  as  in  undisturbed  fa  milk 

In  general,  it  has  been  the  experience  at  Michael  Reese  that  the  moth 
of  a  distuibed  child  explains  that  the  child  is  rebellious  toward  h 
ideas-an  explanation  that  the  Michael  Reese  psychiatrists  are  inclined  1 
disbelieve.  In  describing  behavior  as  rebellious,  they  suggest,  perhaps 
mother  is  excessively  suspicious  or  distrustful.  Or  perhaps  she  has  bcc 
making  inappropriate  demands  on  the  adolescent.  Or  perhaps,  as  in  til 
study,  the  lines  of  communications  have  broken  down :  the  adolescent  h; 
not  known  where  the  paient  stood  and  the  parent  has  not  known  wliei 
the  adolescent  stood. 

Lack  of  communication,  the  investigators  repoit,  may  apply  pailiculai 
ly  to  adolescents  who  are  delinquent  but  not  psychotic.  In  the  prcsen 
study,  in  any  event,  such  teenagers  said  they  had  been  tiying  through  Ihoi 
behavior  to  foice  open  the  communications  at  home,  particularly  vvitl 
their  mothers. 


III.  Normality  in  Young  College  Men 

Several  years  ago,  as  part  of  a  research  pioject  on  how  the  botlj 
lesponds  when  emotions  are  repressed,  Dr.  Grinker  had  occasion  to  stud) 
65  normal  young  men.  They  were  freshmen  at  George  Williams  College  in 
Chicago  where  the  basic  goal  is  "to  provide  professional  education  foi 
Christian  leadership,  primarily  for  Young  Men's  Christian  Associations," 
but  where  "men  and  women  of  all  faiths  and  races  who  seek  to  prepaid 
themselves  in  a  Christian  atmosphere.  .  ."  are  welcomed. 

About  half  the  group  had  been  selected  as  normal  on  the  basis  of 
various  personality  tests;  the  others  were  judged  normal  on  the  basis  of  u 
psychiatric  interview  and  answeis  to  an  extensive  questionnaire. 

After  interviewing  the  selected  students,  Dr.  Grinker's  findings  were 
startling.  "Here  was  a  type  of  young  man  I  had  not  met  before  in  my  role 
of  psychiatrist  and  rarely  in  my  personal  life,"  he  repoited.  "On  the 
surface  they  were  free  from  psychotic,  neurotic,  or  disabling  personality 
tiaits.  It  seemed  that  I  had  encountered  some  mentally  'healthy'  men  who 
presented  a  unique  opportunity  for  study."  As  the  investigation  continued 
he  came  to  feel  that  the  entire  student  body  enjoyed  unusual  health. 

Typically  the  healthy  young  men  in  the  study  came  from  small-  or 
medium-sized  Midwestern  cities.  Their  fathers  were  semiskilled  or  while- 
collar  workeis  eauiing  a  little  more  than  $5,000.  The  parents  had  been 
loving  but  strict.  Family  quarrels,  if  any,  had  been  generally  over  money. 
The  boys  had  gone  to  work  early.  They  had  loved  sports.  They  had  had 
rigorous  training  in  religion. 

Factors  emphasized  by  Dr.  Grinker  as  having  contributed  to  the  boy's 
mental  health  include; 

1,  Sound  physical  health  from  birth  onward. 

2.  Average  rather  than  high  intelligence. 

244 


3.  Warm  relationship  with  both  parents. 

4.  Parental  agreement  about  bringing  up  children-including  the  setting 
of  definite  and  understood  limitations  on  behavior. 

5     Reasonable  and  consistent  punishment. 
6.   Sound  early  religious  tiaining. 
7     Part-time  jobs  when  young. 

8.  Strong  identification  with  the  father. 

9.  A  viewpoint  (picked  up  at  home,  at  church,  in  boys'  clubs,  at  the 
Y.M.C.A.)  that  sees  the  world  as  calling  for  action,  not  introspection:  a 
person  does  something  about  problems. 

10.  Ideals  centered  on  doing  the  job  well,  doing  good,  being  liked, 
achieving   contentment    and    sociability,   and    succeeding  at   what  one 
chooses  to  do  nithei  than  striving  foi  either  social  or  economic  prestige. 

The  investigator  divided  his  healthy  young  men  into  three  groups—the 
very  well  adjusted,  the  fairly  well  adjusted,  and  the  marginally  adjusted— 
and  then  studied  differences  in  their  background  and  behavior.  In  general, 
the  parents  of  the  very  well  adjusted  group  had  more  often  been  in  agree- 
ment about  then  children's  unbiinging,  had  more  openly  expressed  their 
affection  for  each  other,  and  had  shown  a  less  rigid  concern  over  such 
problems  as  dating,  smoking,  and  chinking.  The  motheis  had  been  waimcr, 
closei,  and  more  relaxed,  and  more  of  the  fathers  had  given  all  the  love 
the  students  recalled  having  wanted.  The  very  well  adjusted  individuals 
had  done  better  in  school  and  had  been  more  active  socially  during  adoles- 
cence. They  were  more  specific  than  the  others  about  what  they  wanted 
to  do-go  into  Y.M.C./1  work,  most  of  them -after  college.  They  were  less 
frequently  anxious,  embarrassed,  or  depressed.  When  they  weie  angered 
they  tended  to  speak  out-  the  marginally  adjusted  ones,  to  keep  quiet. 

Even  members  of  the  very  well  adjusted  group  have  had  and  do  have 
problems.  Dr.  Grinker  reports,  "Like  anyone  living,  they  have  had 
conflicts,  established  defenses,  and  have  had  to  sacrifice  potential  assets  in 
tlie  process  of  adjustment."  He  notes  in  particular  a  nairowed  range  of 
interests  and  a  tendency  toward  some  anxiety  about  failing.  But  in  general 
they  and  the  others  who  were  studied  work  and  play  well,  cope 
realistically  with  experiences  that  rouse  them  emotionally,  and  have  had 
warm,  human  relationships  with  parents,  teachers,  friends,  and  girls.  They 
also  feel  good,  and  have  hopes  for  the  future— among  them,  "doing  the 
best  I  can." 

When  he  describes  these  young  men  to  social  and  professional  groups, 
Dr.  Grinker  is  often  told,  "Those  boys  are  sick;  they  have  no  ambition," 
He  disagrees  because  he  thinks  "doing  the  best  I  can"  is  an  ambition. 

Intense  commitment  to  change,  the  investigator  says,  may  in  itself  be 
one  of  the  elements  in  neurosis-building.  Neither  the  men  in  this  study  nor 
their  parents  have  shown  much  interest  in  moving  fast;  they  go  ahead  at  a 
pace  that  does  not  overstrain.  They  are  not  creative,  not  explorative;  to 
many  persons  they  might  appear  dull.  But  Dr.  Grinker  believes  that  they 
and  people  like  them  give  our  society  "a  solid  steady  core  of  stability." 

The  investigator  observes  that  what  he  considers  mental  health  or 
normality  in  these  young  men  is  of  one  type  and  that  research  among 

245 


many  kinds  of  populations  is  necessary  to  delineate  other  types  and  find 
what  they  have  in  common. 


Research  Granf   MH4870 

References- 

Grmker,   Roy   R.,  Sr.   (with  the  collaboration  of  Roy  R.  Grinker,  Ji.,  and  Jolm 

Timberlake).  "Mentally  healthy"  young  males  (homochtes).  Archives  of  General 

Psychiatry,  6(6),  1962. 
Offer,  Daniel  and   Sabshin,  Melvm.  The  psychiatiist  and   the  normal  adolesctiiit. 

Archives  of  General  Psychiatry,  9  (5),  1963. 
Offer,  Daniel,   Sabshin,  Melvin,  and  Marcus,  David.  Clinical  evaluation  of  uorm-il 

adolescents,  Presented  to  American  Psychiatric  Association,  May  1964.  A  men  fan 

Journal  of  Psychiatry,  121(9),  1965, 


246 


Investigator: 

Albert  Bandura,  Ph.  D. 

Stanford  University 
Palo  Alto,  Calif. 

Prepared  by, 

Clarissa  Wittenberg 

One  of  the  fundamental  means  by  which  human  behavior  is  acquired 
and  modified  involves  vicarious  learning.  Both  children  and  adults  learn 
modes  and  standards  to  a  great  extent  by  observing  the  behavior  of  others. 
A  great  deal  of  attention  has  been  focused  in  this  country  upon  the 
television  industry  as  this  fact  has  become  more  and  moie  apparent.  In 
addition  to  television,  motion  pictures,  books,  and  other  reading  material 
are  visual  media  effective  in  producing  vicarious,  as  opposed  to  diiectly 
experienced,  learning.  The  variety  represented  by  television  alone  demon- 
strates the  difficulty  of  making  simple  judgments  about  the  effect  on  the 
viewer.  The  variations  in  viewers  and  the  surrounding  ciicumslanccs  in 
which  they  see  the  television  again  points  out  the  complexity.  The  most 
insistent  public  focus  on  TV  has  been  with  regard  to  violence  and  its  part 
in  the  violence  observed  in  our  society.  An  investigation  of  violence  per  se 
seems  less  likely  to  yield  information  on  the  underlying  process  of 
influence  than  is  the  study  of  the  visual  media  with  regard  to  its  impact  on 
learning  in  general.  Dr.  Albert  Bandura  has  spent  ovci  ten  years  studying 
the  effects  of  such  media  upon  children  and  adults  in  a  variety  of  sit- 
uations. His  work  yields  the  definite  finding  that  such  media  are  powerful 
methods  of  teaching  and  important  sources  of  influence,  but  that  many 
lessons  may  be  taught  and  they  may  not  be  the  obvious  ones.  It  becomes 
obvious  that  the  environment  in  which  the  media  is  viewed  is  very  impor- 
tant. 

This  research  has  demonstrated  the  stimulation  of  aggressive  behavior 
by  the  viewing  of  aggressive  acts  on  a  screen.  Variables  have  then  been 
explored  with  attention  given  to  the  characteristics  of  the  model  and  the 
attributes  of  the  observer,  and  the  consequences  accompanying  the 
demonstrated  patterns  of  behavior,  The  difference  between  acquisition 
and  spontaneous  performance  of  aggressive  acts  has  been  examined. 
Further  research  has  been  done  with  the  use  of  the  visual  media  as 
therapeutic  tools  to  relieve  longstanding  and  serious  phobias  and  to 
improve  the  social  adjustment  of  withdrawn  children.  The  common  thread 
throughout  the  research  of  Doctor  Bandura  and  his  associates  is  the 
concept  of  observational  learning  and  the  effectivity  of  the  modeling 
process. 

247 


Doctor  Banduia  states  that  research  bearing  on  modeling  processes 
demonstrates  that,  unlike  the  relatively  slow  process  of  trial -and -err  or 
learning,  patterns  of  behavioi  are  lapidly  acquired  observationally  in  large 
segments  or  in  their  enthety.  The  extent  of  this  form  of  learning  can  be 
seen  in  children's  play  when  they  reproduce  parental  behavior,  including 
the  appropriate  mannerisms,  voice  inflections,  and  attitudes.  This  process 
in  a  more  general  way  is  leferred  to  as  "identification." 

Doctor  Bandura  became  involved  in  this  subject  in  1958  when  lie 
conducted  with  Richard  Walters,  research  on  the  family  conditions  which 
gave  rise  to  extreme  aggression  in  children.  The  focus  was  the  adolescent 
from  the  "good  home"  who  became  antisocial  and  delinquent.  Although  a 
great  deal  of  research  had  been  done  about  the  effects  of  poor  and  adveise 
family  and  social  conditions,  not  much  had  been  done  to  explain  the 
reasons  why  affluent  young  men  were  becoming  delinquent  and  antisocial. 
Families  were  selected  who  looked  well  integrated  and  socially  we]l 
adjusted,  but  whose  children  were  being  followed  by  the  probation 
department  in  the  San  Francisco  area.  A  matched  control  group  was  also 
interviewed.  Two  cential  factors  emerged.  Many  parents  of  the  delinquent 
boys  weie  models  for  antisocial  attitudes  and  aggressive  behavior  despite 
their  smooth  social  exterior.  A  second  pattern  was  that  the  parents, 
especially  the  fatheis,  often  would  not  permit  aggression  towards 
themselves  but  would  encourage  and  rewaid  their  son  in  fights  outside  tlie 
family  01  defend  the  boy's  right  to  "raise-."  In  sharp  contrast,  the  non- 
delinquent  boys  were  encouraged  more  to  defend  themselves  with  their 
ideas  or  in  the  nonphysical  spheres.  Aggression  of  a  physical  type  was 
consistently  discouraged  through  nonpunitive  means  in  these  families. 
These  boys,  who  were  not  on  probation,  had  been  taught  through 
example  and  precept  a  different  way  of  solving  their  interpersonal 
problems  than  the  aggressive  boys. 

Another  incident  occurred  that  dramatized  the  influence  of  demon- 
stration or  modeling.  In  1961  the  San  Francisco  Chronicle  reported  that  a 
boy  had  been  seriously  knifed  during  a  reenactment  of  switchblade 
fight  the  boys  had  seen  the  previous  evening  on  a  televised  rerun  of  the 
James  Dean  movie,  Rebel  Without  a  Cause.  This  was  a  vivid  illustration  of 
the  imitation  of  film  stimulation  and  stirred  considerable  speculation.  The 
form  of  the  aggression  had  been  so  clearly  shaped  by  the  film  that  it  gnvc 
rise  to  the  idea  that  aggression  viewed  through  pictorial  media  may  be 
influential  in  shaping  the  form  of  aggression  when  the  person  is  in  a 
provocative  situation.  The  importance  of  the  visual  media  in  stimulation 
or  instigating  aggression  also  became  a  focus  on  this  research. 


Transmission  of  Aggression 

One  set  of  experiments  was  designed  in  1961  to  determine  the  extent 
to  which  aggression  could  be  transmitted  to  children  through  exposure  to 
aggressive  adult  models.  In  this  early  experiment  children  observed  an 
adult  who  exhibited  relatively  unusual  forms  of  physical  and  verbal 

248 


aggression  towards  a  large  inflated  plastic  "Bobo"  doll.  A  second  group 
watched  a  very  subdued  and  inhibited  model.  The  control  group  saw  no 
model  at  all.  Half  the  children  in  each  experimental  condition  saw  models 
of  the  same  sex  and  half  observed  the  opposite  sex.  Later  the  children 
were  mildly  frustrated  by  having  toys  restiicted  for  their  use  and  then 
their  behavior  was  recorded  in  a  new  situation  where  they  could  behave 
either  aggressively  or  nonaggressively.  The  results  showed  that  exposure 
to  aggressive  models  heightened  the  children's  aggressive  responses  to  sub- 
sequent frustration  in  new  settings  in  which  the  model  was  absent. 

In  1963  this  investigation  was  extended  by  Banduia,  Ross  and  Ross. 
The  effects  of  real  life  models  and  filmed  models  were  compaied.  The 
children  in  this  project,  as  well  as  in  many  of  these  studies,  were  diawn 
from  the  Stanford  University  Nursery  School.  In  this  study  they  ranged 
from  35  to  69  months  of  age,  with  a  mean  age  of  52  months.  There  were 
48  boys  and  48  girls  who  took  part.  Two  adults,  a  male  and  a  female, 
served  as  models.  A  female  experimenter  conducted  the  study  with  all  of 
the  children.  A  "Bobo"  doll  was  again  used  as  the  subject  of  aggression. 

One  group  of  children  observed  real  models  in  the  room  with  them, 
behaving  aggressively  toward  the  doll.  A  second  gioup  saw  a  film  of  the 
same  models  performing  the  same  acts.  A  third  group  saw  a  cartoon  of  an 
aggressive  figure.  The  control  group  was  not  exposed  to  any  of  these 
stimulations.  Again  half  of  the  children  saw  models  of  theii  same  sex. 
These  children  had  previously  been  rated  in  terms  of  their  normal  ag- 
gressive behavior  by  their  nuiscry  school  teachers  and  they  were  matched 
to  the  control  group  on  this  basis. 

The  children  who  saw  the  real  life  aggression  weie  asked  into  a  room 
and  invited  to  join  a  game.  The  child  was  shown  a  table  with  a  variety  of 
activities.  The  model  also  worked  at  a  small  table  doing  tinker  toys.  Then 
the  model  turned  to  the  "Bobo"  doll  and  kicked  it  about  the  room,  sat  on 
it  and  punched  it  in  the  head,  pummeled  it,  and  hit  it  with  a  mallet  among 
other  things.  The  sequence  of  acts  was  repeated  three  times  and  was 
accompanied  by  verbally  aggressive  comments  such  as  "Sock  him  in  the 
nose  *  *  V  "hit  him  down  '*  *  *,"  "Pow."  These  acts  were  not  those 
usually  performed  spontaneously  by  children  with  a  "Bobo"  doll. 
Although  the  doll  is  designed  to  be  hit,  the  usual  play  involves  poking  it  or 
trying  to  knock  it  over  rather  than  "beating  it  up."  The  main  inteicst  of 
the  research  was  not  whether  the  children  hit  the  doll  but  whether  they 
adopted  the  unusual  modes  of  aggression  demonstrated  by  the  adults. 

The  movie  sequence  was  identical  except  it  was  presented  on  film. 

The  cartoon  sequence  was  presented  in  a  TV  console  and  the  ex- 
perimenter introduced  it  as  a  color  TV  cartoon  program.  A  film  was  then 
presented  of  a  female  model  costumed  as  a  cat  performing  against  a 
brightly  colored  and  fantastic  setting.  A  title  and  a  picture  of  a  stage 
introduced  the  production.  The  cat  figure  then  performed  the  same  acts 
with  the  "Bobo"  doll.  Music  was  played  accompanying  the  film. 

Fallowing  the  exposure,  the  children  were  tested  for  the  amount  of 
imitative  and  nonimitative  aggression  in  a  different  experimental  setting 
without  the  presence  of  models. 

249 


In  ordei  to  clearly  differentiate  the  exposure  and  test  situatio 
subjects  were  tested  for  the  amount  of  imitative  learning  in  a  diffen 
experimental  room  which  was  set  off  fiom  the  main  nursery  scln 
biiilding. 

The  childien,  both  control  and  experimental  groups,  were  mile 
frustrated  before  they  were  biought  to  the  test  room  by  having  t 
children  begin  to  play  with  attractive  toys  and  then  being  told  that  th 
were  the  experimenter's  best  toys  and  that  she  was  saving  them.  T 
childien  weie  then  taken  to  the  testing  room  and  the  experimenter  stay 
with  them,  but  did  papeiwork  off  to  one  side. 

The  testing  room  contained  a  variety  of  toys,  some  of  which  could 
used  for  imitative  or  nonimitative  aggressive  acts,  and  others  which  te 
to  elicit  predominantly  nonaggressive  forms  of  behavior.  The  aggressive 
oiiented  toys  included  a  "Bobo"  doll,  a  mallet  and  pegboaid,  dart  giti 
etc.  The  othets  included  a  tea  set,  ciayons  and  paper,  a  ball,  two  dolls,  e 
Play  mateiial  was  also  arranged  in  such  a  way  as  to  eliminate  any  var 
tions  in  behavior  due  to  mere  placement. 

The  subject  spent  20  minutes  in  the  testing  room  during  which  time 
was  observed  through  a  one-way  mirror  and  his  behavior  was  rated.  Tl 
20-minute  session  was  divided  into  5-second  intervals,  and  so  a  subject  w 
scored  240  times.  The  judges  reached  high  levels  of  reliability  in  thi 
scoring.  The  following  response  measures  were  obtained;  imitati 
aggression;  partially  imitative  responses;  mallet  aggression;  sitting  < 
"Bobo"  doll;  nonimitative  aggression;  aggressive  gunplay. 


Results 

Exposure  to  aggressive  models  increased  the  probability  that  subjec 
will  respond  aggressively  when  instigated  on  later  occasions.  Furth 
analysis  shows  that  subjects  who  viewed  the  real  life  models  do  not  diff 
from  those  who  viewed  the  filmed  or  TV  models  in  total  aggressivene; 
but  all  thiee  experimental  groups  expressed  significantly  more  no 
imitative  aggressive  behavior  than  the  control  subjects. 

The  exposure  to  aggressive  models  is  a  highly  effective  method  f 
shaping  subjects'  aggressive  responses.  Experimental  subjects  displayed 
high  level  of  imitative  physical  and  verbally  aggressive  acts  whereas  contr 
subjects  rarely  behaved  in  these  novel  aggressive  ways.  Thus  exposure 
aggressive  models  not  only  reduced  children's  inhibitions  over  aggressi1 
behavior  that  they  had  previously  learned,  but  also  taught  them  new  wa; 
of  aggressing, 

A  prediction  had  been  made  that  imitation  is  positively  related  to  tl 
reality  cues  of  the  model  and  this  was  only  partially  supported.  Whi 
subjects  who  observed  the  real-life  aggressive  models  exhibit* 
significantly  more  imitative  aggression  than  subjects  who  viewed  tl 
cartoon  model,  the  live  and  film,  and  the  film  and  cartoon  models  i 
creased  nonimitative  aggression  in  the  children  to  the  same  degree.  Da 
indicated  that  of  the  three  experimental  conditions,  exposure  to  fnimai 


on  film  portraying  aggression  was  the  most  influential  in  eliciting  and 
shaping  aggressive  behavior. 


The  Effect  of  the  Sex  of  the  Model 

The  boys  exhibited  more  total  aggression  than  girls,  more  imitative 
aggression,  more  aggressive  gunplay  and  more  nonimitative  aggressive 
behavior.  The  girls,  for  instance,  were  more  likely  to  sit  on  a  "Bobo"  doll 
and  lefrain  from  punching  it. 

Subjects  who  were  exposed  to  male  models  as  compared  to  female 
models  expressed  significantly  more  aggressive  gunplay.  The  most  marked 
cHffeiences  in  aggressive  gunplay  were,  however,  found  between  the  girls 
who  had  been  exposed  to  the  female  model  and  males  who  had  observed 
the  male  model.  The  girls  who  saw  the  female  model  tended  to  reproduce 
more  partially  imitative  acts  than  the  boys  who  saw  the  male  model  and 
were  more  likely  to  repioduce  the  large:  actions. 

The  sex  of  the  child  and  the  sex  of  the  model  have  an  effect  upon  the 
degree  of  influence  of  the  models,  and  this  influence  is  determined  in  part 
by  the  sex  appropriateness  of  the  model's  behavior. 

Another  section  of  this  experiment  dealt  with  the  possibility  of 
cathartic  action  upon  the  viewing  of  aggressive  film  material.  The  subjects 
were  first  frustrated  and  then  provided  with  an  opportunity  to  view  an 
aggressive  film  following  which  their  overt  or  fantasied  aggression  was 
measured.  Many  parents  and  educators  encourage  hyperaggressive  childien 
to  participate  in  aggressive  recreational  activities,  to  view  highly  aggressive 
TV  programs,  and  to  be  aggressive  in  therapeutic  settings  in  order  to 
"discharge"  their  aggression.  Bandura's  work  and  the  work  of  other  in- 
vestigators demonstrate  that  the  provision  of  aggressive  models  and  the 
inadvertent  reinforcement  of  aggression  which  occurs  in  these  situations 
act  to  encourage  aggiessive  tendencies  rather  than  dissipating  them.  On 
the  other  hand,  providing  aggressive  children  with  examples  of  alternative, 
constructive  ways  of  coping  with  frustration  can  be  very  successful  in 
helping  them  modify  their  destructive  behavior  patterns.  Already 
frustrated  children  show  more  aggressive  behavior  after  viewing  live  or 
filmed  aggression  than  do  frustrated  children  who  are  not  shown  a  film, 
The  filmed  aggression  does  not  fill  theii  need,  nor  does  it  diminish  their 
aggressive  tendencies. 

The  view  that  social  learning  of  aggression  through  exposure  to  ag- 
gressive film  content  is  adopted  by  only  deviant  children,  also  finds  little 
support  in  Doctor  Bandura's  research.  The  children  who  participated  in 
this  experiment  were  all  considered  normal;  yet  88  percent  of  the  subjects 
in  the  "Real-Life"  and  in  the  "Human  Film"  condition,  and  79  percent  of 
those  in  the  "Cartoon"  condition,  exhibited  varying  degrees  of  imitative 
aggressive  behavior.  In  assessing  the  possible  influence  of  televised  stimula- 
tion on  behavior,  it  is  important  to  distinguish  between  learning  and  overt 
performance.  Although  children  may  learn  whole  patterns  of  behavior  by 

251 


watching  TV,  they  do  not  ordinarily  perform  indiscriminately  the  t» 
havior  of  televised  characters,  even  those  who  they  regard  as  highly  attnn 
tive.  The  responses  of  the  parents  appear  to  be  very  important  in  discou 
aging  overt  imitation.  The  investigators  stress  that  the  behavior  is  learnct 
however,  even  if  parental  disapproval  inhibits  it  being  performed  and 
may  be  elicited  on  future  occasions.  Indeed,  recent  research  demonstn 
tions  show  that  children  will  not  exhibit  disapproved  aggression  in  th 
presence  of  the  prohibitive  adult,  but  that  they  are  inclined  to  perfori 
such  behavior  when  the  disapproving  adult  is  absent. 

Children  who  had  been  previously  rated  as  more  aggressive  than  th 
others  by  their  teachers  did  not  differ  in  their  aggressive  reactions  in  th 
experimental  setting. 

The  investigatois  have  formulated  a  theory  of  social  learning  of  aj 
gression  (Bandura  and  Walters,  1959)  that  would  suggest  that  most  of  th 
responses  utilized  to  hurt  or  injure  others,  such  as  kicking  or  hitting,  wcr 
learned  as  exploratory  asocial  acts.  For  instance,  the  infant  who  learns  I 
control  his  legs  and  kick  is  exercising  and  exploring  his  own  movement! 
but  not  being  aggressive.  When  frustrated,  however,  he  may  call  on  thi 
response  as  one  that  can  express  his  intense  feelings,  and  then  the  kickin 
becomes  involved  in  social  interaction.  On  the  basis  of  this  theory,  i 
would  be  predicted  that  the  aggressive  responses  acquhed  imitntivcly 
while  not  necessarily  for  aggressive  goals  in  the  experimental  scttinj 
would  be  utilized  to  serve  such  purposes  in  other  social  settings.  It  wonli 
also  be  predicted  that  children  in  the  experimental  settings  would  use  thi 
behavior  aggressively  more  frequently  than  children  in  the  control  groups 

These  previously  mentioned  experiments  were  primarily  designed  ti 
measure  the  extent  to  which  children  learn  by  observing  the  aggressive 
action  of  adults.  A  second  major  question  is  whether  exposure  to  ag 
gressive  models  influences  the  harshness  with  which  people  treat  otheis 
To  study  interpersonal  expiession  and  aggressive  behavior  requires  stndic 
in  which  people  are  provided  with  opportunities  to  behave  punitive]) 
toward  another  person  after  viewing  aggressive  or  nonaggrcssive  models,  / 
study  conducted  in  Doctor  Bandura *s  laboratory  by  Donald  Hnrtmat 
reveals  that  aggressive  models  not  only  foster  learning  of  aggrossivi 
behavior,  but  can  also  increase  interpersonal  aggression. 

The  catharsis  hypothesis  has  generally  assumed  that  viewing  aggrcssior 
reduces  aggressive  tendencies  in  observers  if  they  experience  anger  at  tlu 
time  of  exposure,  but  that  it  may  increase  aggression  in  nonangcrctl 
viewers.  To  test  this  idea,  Hartman  conducted  an  experiment  thai 
proceeded  in  the  following  manner.  One  group  of  delinquent  adolescent; 
underwent  an  anger-arousing  experience,  while  a  second  group  had  an 
essentially  neutral  experience.  The  boys  then  observed  one  of  three 
movies.  In  the  control  film  two  boys  engage  in  an  active  but  cooperative 
basketball  game,  whereas  in  the  other  two  films  the  boys  get  into  an 
argument  that  develops  into  a  fist  fight.  The  instrumental-aggression  film 
focuses  on  the  behavior  of  the  attacker,  including  his  angry  facia!  ex- 
pressions, flying  fists,  foot  thrusts,  and  hostile  remarks.  The  pain-cues  film 

252 


focuses  almost  exclusively  on  the  reactions  of  the  victim  as  he  is 
pummeled  and  kicked  by  his  opponent. 

Major  obstacles  arise  in  the  study  of  interpeisonal  aggression  because  a 
socially  significant  measure  would  involve  injurious  behavior  which  cannot 
be  used  for  humane  and  ethical  reasons.  This  major  obstacle  has  been 
overcome  by  several  researches  by  creating  a  situation  in  which  one 
person  can  administer  shocks  of  differing  intensities  and  durations  of  his 
own  choosing  to  another  person.  However,  the  electrodes  are  not  con- 
nected to  the  victim  so  that  he  in  fact  docs  not  suffer  any  pain.  After 
viewing  the  films,  the  boys  in  Hartman's  study  were  provided  with  oppor- 
tunities to  shock  a  victim.  The  intensity  and  the  duration  of  the  shocks 
administered  were  recorded. 

Boys  who  had  observed  either  the  aggressive  acts  or  the  pain-cues  films 
selected  significantly  highei  shock  levels,  both  under  angered  and 
nonangered  conditions  than  boys  who  watched  the  control  film. 
Moreover,  angered  viewers  behaved  moie  punitively  than  nonangered 
viewers  following  exposure  to  the  aggressive  films,  a  finding  that  is 
directly  counter  to  the  prediction  of  the  usual  catharsis  hypothesis.  Boys 
behaved  most  aggressively  when  they  were  angered  and  witnessed  another 
person  beaten  severely. 


Vicarious  Reinforcement  and  Learning 

In  1963  a  study  was  reported  by  Banduia,  Ross  and  Ross  which 
explored  the  issue  of  vicarious  leinforcement;  that  is,  the  changes  in  the 
behavior  of  observers  resulting  from  witnessing  the  consequences  ex- 
perienced by  others.  In  this  study  >  nursery  school  children  witnessed  a 
variety  of  situations.  The  prime  issue  was  whether  or  not  they  viewed  an 
aggressive  model  being  rewarded  for  his  acts.  The  major  finding  was  that 
children  who  witnessed  the  aggressive  model  rewarded  showed  more 
imitative  aggression,  and  preferred  to  emulate  the  successful  aggressor 
than  children  who  observed  the  aggressive  model  who  was  punished.  This 
last  group  both  failed  to  reproduce  his  behavior  and  rejected  him  as  a 
model.  Control  over  aggression  was  vicariously  transmitted  to  the  boys  by 
the  administration  of  negative  responses  to  the  model  and  to  the  girls  by 
the  presentation  of  socially  incompatible  examples  of  behavior. 

Interviews  with  the  children  at  the  completion  of  the  experiment  dis- 
closed that  although  children  in  the  aggression-rewarded  conditions  voiced 
disapproval  while  they  watched  the  acts  that  they  nevertheless  emulated 
his  behavior  on  the  basis  of  its  success.  The  key  issue  was  that  they 
admired  the  power  the  model  gained  over  reward  resources  through  his 
icprehensible  behavior.  The  investigators  noted  that  the  children  stated 
that  physical  aggression  and  forceful  confiscation  of  the  property  of 
others  is  wrong  and  they  criticized  the  model  for  doing  it.  Therefore, 
when  these  same  children  later  copied  this  type  of  behavior,  they  can  be 
expected  to  experience  considerable  conflict  and  discomfort.  They  did 
not  resolve  this  conflict  by  praising  violence,  but  tended  to  do  it  by 

253 


criticizing  the  victim.  They  viewed  the  victim  as  weak  or  provocative, 
ungenerous  or  unshaimg,  and  thereby  in  a  sense  "bringing  it  on  himself." 
In  situations  where  the  aggressive  model  was  punished,  even  when  the 
victim  was  quite  provocative,  the  victim  was  not  criticized  and  the  ag- 
gressor was  considered  bad,  Successful  "pay  off"  of  aggression  rather  than 
its  intrinsic  desirability  served  to  stimulate  imitation. 

The  implications  of  this  finding  in  terms  of  the  attitudinal  and 
behavioral  effects  of  television  would  indicate  that  successful  hostile  ag- 
gression would  outweigh  even  the  previously  established  values  of  right  or 
wrong  for  the  viewer  This  study  involves  only  a  single  aggressive  incident 
that  was  rewarded  or  punished.  In  most  televised  programs  the  "bad  guys" 
gain  contiol  over  important  lesources  and  win  considerable  social  and 
mateiial  rewards  through  aggressive  acts,  and  punishment,  if  any,  is 
delayed,  as  Dr.  Bandura  says,  "until  the  last  commercial."  Many  episodes 
which  are  antisocial  and  "pay  off"  are  viewed  before  the  punishment 
occurs. 

Bandura  and  his  associates  find  that  feai  of  a  punitive  or  aggressive 
model  is  not  a  necessary  factor  in  identification  and  adoption  of  aggressive 
behavior.  The  success  of  the  aggressive  act  rather  than  the  fear  of  the 
aggressive  agent  is  seen  as  more  influential.  This  has  relevance  to  the 
concept  suggested  by  Freud  of  "identification  with  the  aggressor,"  which 
postulates  that  a  perceived  threat  by  a  punitive  agent  is  the  primary 
motivating  force  in  the  assumption  of  aggressive  traits. 


Social  Power,  Status  and  Identification 

Although  it  is  often  assumed  that  social  behavior  is  learned  and 
modified  through  direct  reward  and  punishment  of  responses,  informal 
observation  and  studies  suggest  that  the  "power"  of  the  individual 
involved  may  also  be  influential.  A  child  who  perceives  his  mother  as  a 
prime  source  of  rewards  in  the  family  may  identify  with  her  rather  than 
with  the  father  who  he  may  see  as  occupying  a  subordinate  position,  and 
even  compete  with  him  for  rewards. 

A  study  was  devised  to  set  up  conditions  with  nursery  school  children 
and  female  and  male  models  to  reproduce  possible  family  constellations  of 
power  and  reward  structures.  One  of  the  adults  assumed  the  role  of 
controller  of  a  fabulous  collection  of  toys  and  offered  to  go  shopping  for 
such  highly  desirable  items  as  two-wheel  bicycles  for  the  children.  The 


Models  who  were  seen  as  having  the  power  to  reward  elicited  twice  as 
much  imitative  behavior  as  models  who  were  perceived  by  the  children  as 
possessing  no  contiol  over  the  rewarding  resources.  Power  inversions  on 
the  part  of  the  male  and  female  models  produced  cross-sex  differences, 
particularly  in  girls.  It  was  found  a  differential  readiness  existed  between 
boys  and  gills  in  the  willingness  to  imitate  behavior  by  an  opposite  sex 
model.  Boys  showed  a  decided  preference  for  the  masculine  role,  whereas 
ambivalence  and  a  masculine  role  preference  were  widespread  among  the 
girls.  The  investigators  suggest  that  these  findings  probably  reflect  both 
the  differential  cultural  tolerance  for  cross-sex  behavior  displayed  by 
males  and  females,  and  the  privileged  status  and  relatively  gieater  positive 
reinforcement  of  masculine  role  behavior  in  our  society. 

The  research  team  further  suggests  that  although  failure  to  develop 
sex-appropriate  behavior  has  received  consideiable  attention  and  is 
often  assumed  to  be  established  and  maintained  by  concepts  of  depend- 
ency, psychosexual  threat  and  anxiety,  external  social  variables  may  also 
be  important.  For  instance,  the  distribution  of  the  rewarding  power 
within  a  family  may  be  very  important.  Although  the  small  child  has  great 
contact  with  this  mother  he  also  has  ample  time  to  observe  his  father's 
behavior.  Also  children  do  not  adopt  "wholesale"  the  tiaits  of  one  model. 
A  child  exhibits  a  relatively  novel  mix  of  behavior  in  his  own  repertoire. 

The  makeup  of  the  family  constellation  is  also  important.  This  research 
shows  that  in  a  three-person  group,  for  instance,  if  one  person  is  denied 
access  to  rewards,  the  others  may  experience  negative  evaluations  of  the 
rewarding  model  and  thereby  decrease  his  impact  as  a  modeling  stimulus. 
The  introduction  of  each  new  person  and  his  treatment  at  the  hands  of 
the  model  may  produce  new  shifts  in  the  relationships. 


Reinforcement  by  Self-Approval 

People  tend  to  set  for  themselves  certain  standards  of  behavior  and 
respond  to  their  own  actions  in  self-rewarding  and  self-punishing  ways  in 
accordance  with  their  self-imposed  demands.  This  is  a  major  difference  in 
human  and  animal  learning  studies.  Even  children  can  decide  whether 
their  own  performance  is  creditable  or  not.  Speculation  about  how 
children  develop  these  internal  standards  was  transformed  into  an  ex- 
periment. The  children  were  given  the  opportunity  to  observe  models 
performing,  and  then  permitted  to  evaluate  their  attainments  according  to 
high  or  low  standards  and  reward  themselves  accordingly .  It  was  predicted 
that  children  tend  to  adopt  the  standards  of  self-reward  exhibited  by  the 
models  they  observed,  but  that  children  in  the  control  group  who  saw  no 
model  would  have  no  consistent  pattern  of  self-reinforcement.  It  was  also 
predicted  that  the  subjects  would  adopt  the  self-reinforcement  patterns  of 
the  same  sex  model  to  a  greater  degree  than  that  of  a  model  of  the 
opposite  sex.  It  was  also  predicted  that  children  would  match  the  self- 
reinforcement  patterns  of  adult  models  more  closely  than  those  of  peers. 

This  study  reported  by  Bandura  and  Kupers  had  a  group  of  boys  and 
girls  from  a  summer  recreation  program  as  subjects.  It  was  designed  to  be 

255 


an  investigation  of  the  transmission  of  standards  of  self-reward.  The 
children  ranged  from  7  to  9  years  in  age.  Adult  and  child  models  per- 
formed a  bowling  game  in  which  they  adopted  either  high  achievement 
standards  or  a  low  standard  for  self-ieward.  On  games  in  which  the  models 
attained  their  standard,  they  praised  themselves  and  treated  themselves  to 
candy;  but  when  their  attainments  fell  short  of  their  adopted  standards, 
they  appeared  self-critical.  Later,  the  children  who  had  observed,  played 
the  same  game  alone  and  the  scores  for  which  they  rewarded  themselves 
were  recorded.  The  control  children  saw  no  models  at  all. 

Children  who  saw  no  models  or  who  saw  models  with  low  standaids 
tended  to  reward  themselves  generously  following  a  mediocre  attainment. 
Children,  who  saw  models  set  high  standards  for  self-rewards,  rewarded 
themselves  sparingly  and  only  when  they  attained  a  superior  performance. 
This  suggests  that  the  behavior  of  the  models  is  influential  in  the  develop- 
ment of  self-control  as  well  as  in  the  transmission  of  standards  for  self- 
rewards.  The  children  tended  to  match  the  patterns  foi  rewards  set  by  the 
adult  models  more  closely  than  those  set  by  peer  models.  The  results 
showed  that  patterns  of  self-reinforcement  can  be  acquired  imitativcly 
through  exposuie  to  models,  without  the  subjects  themselves  being  admin- 
istered any  direct  differential  reinforcement  by  external  agents. 

Another  study  was  devised  to  further  examine  the  formation  of 
personal  standards.  In  the  experiment  previously  described,  the  children 
modeled  their  own  standards  after  those  of  the  model.  It  was  thought  that 
this  was  partly  because  the  performance  scores  had  little  absolute  value 
and  therefore  the  evaluation  of  the  model  served  as  a  primary  basis  for 
judging  what  might  constitute  an  inadequate  or  superior  performance. 
This  study  was  a!so  designed  to  see  if  observeis  would  select  models  who 
were  similar  to  themselves  in  ability  and  reject  those  who  were  markedly 
divergent.  It  was  predicted  that  subjects  would  adopt  the  self- 
reinforcement  standards  of  the  model  whose  ability  or  competence  was 
similar  to  their  own,  They  would  disregard  the  examples  of  those  whose 
accomplishments  were  too  different  from  their  own  and  adopt  a  more 
reasonable  standard  for  themselves.  The  investigators  hypothesized  Hint 
even  low  or  meiely  adequate  performances  by  adults  would  be  highly 
regarded,  and  if  a  child  matched  or  exceeded  the  performance  of  an  adult 
that  it  would  raise  his  own  self-esteem.  It  was  hypothesized  that  children 
attaining  the  achievement  level  of  even  an  inadequate  adult  would  tend  to 
reward  themselves  highly. 

In  this  study,  groups  of  80  boys  and  80  girls  ranging  in  age  from  8  to  1 1 
years  were  given  a  series  of  tasks  and  then  they  were  either  told  that  they 
were  successful  or  not.  Then  they  observed  a  model  displaying  competent, 
superior  or  inferior  performance.  The  superior  model  adopted  an 
exceedingly  high  standard  for  self-reward,  and  the  inferior  model  set  a  low 
one.  The  children  assigned  to  a  control  group  saw  no  models  at  all. 

Dc!sults  showed  that  children  who  observed  inferior  models  tended  to 
t  lower  standards  for  themselves  and  rewarded  themselves  more 
ously  than  children  who  were  exposed  to  more  competent  models 
higher  standards.  Children  tended  to  scale  down  the  achievements  of 


the  adults  to  a  lower  standard  more  commensurate  with  their  own 
abilities. 

This  experiment  also  examined  whether  children's  willingness  to  adhere 
to  high  standards  is  affected  by  their  prior  success  or  failure  experiences. 
Children  who  have  had  failure  experiences  tended  to  reward  themselves 
less  than  their  successful  counterparts,  a  finding  that  was  most  noticeable 
among  children  exposed  to  the  inferior  model.  Control  subjects  who  had 
experienced  failure  displayed  a  higher  rate  of  self-reinforcement  at  lower 
levels  of  performance  than  did  chikhen  who  experienced  past  success.  The 
investigators  suggest  that  under  some  circumstances  self-giatification  may 
primarily  serve  a  therapeutic  rather  than  a  self-congratulatory  function. 
The  same  principle  is  seen  when  a  person  "treats"  himself  to  a  play  or  a 
special  dinner  to  help  himself  over  a  difficult  experience. 

Another  finding  was  that  boys  and  gills  diffeied  significantly  in  the 
frequency  of  verbal  self-piaise,  but  not  in  the  incidence  and  magnitude  of 
self-administered  material  rewards,  such  as  candy.  Boys  weie  more 
generous  in  commending  themselves  for  equivalent  achievements. 


Contiguity  and  Other  Factors 

It  is  often  assumed  that  the  occurrence  of  limitative  or  observational 
learning  is  based  on  the  observer  experiencing  reinforcing  consequences. 
This  does  not  account  for  the  learning  of  imitative  behavior  when  the 
observei  does  not  perform  the  model's  responses  during  the  process  of 
acquisition,  and  where  neither  rewards  nor  punishments  are  given  to  either 
model  or  observer.  It  is  suggested  that  in  these  cases  a  contiguity  theory 
can  best  account  for  observational  learning.  Contiguity  means  that  events 
or  objects  in  a  seiies  or  close  to  each  other  in  time  or  space  become 
associated.  When  an  observer  then  witnesses  a  model  exhibit  a  sequence  of 
responses  the  observer  acquires  through  the  principle  of  contiguous  as- 
sociation of  sensory  events  certain  perceptual  and  symbolic  responses  that 
cue  other  responses  even  aftci  time  has  elapsed. 

Bandura  states  that  the  acquisition  of  matching  responses  may  take 
place  through  contiguity,  whereas  the  reinforcements  administered  to  a 
model  exert  their  major  influence  on  the  performance  of  imitatively 
learned  responses.  Several  of  Bandura's  studies  have  shown  that  even 
children  who  do  not  reproduce  the  aggressive  behavior  of  models  were 
able  to  desciibe  the  behavior  in  great  and  accurate  detail.  When  these 
non performers  were  rewarded  they  would  readily  reproduce  the  modeled 
behavior.  However,  these  children  usually  failed  to  reproduce  the  entire 
behavior  pattern  and  this  indicates  that  factors  other  than  exposure  to 
models  or  contiguity  influence  response  acquisition.  It  appears  that 
observers  attend  to  models  that  arc  most  relevant  to  them.  Both  prior 
experience  and  the  distinctive  qualities  of  the  modeling  example  are  im- 
portant in  determining  the  attention  paid  to  it  by  the  observer. 

Social  behavior  is  generally  highly  complex  and  composed  of  a  large 
number  of  different  behavior  units  combined  in  a  particular  manner. 

257 


Ban  dura  points  out  that  such  responses  are  produced  by  combinations  o 
previously  learned  components  which  themselves  may  be  intricate  units 
The  rate  of  acquisition  of  new  responses  will  then  be  partly  determined  h; 
the  extent  to  which  the  necessary  components  are  contained  in  Hn 
repertoire  of  the  observer.  For  instance,  small  children  may  be  more  iibli 
to  reproduce  motor  behavior  than  verbal  behavior. 

Learning-Pain,  Fear,  and  Other  Emotional  States 

Studies  of  vicarious  emotional  learning  show  that  people  develo} 
emotional  reactions  to  certain  places,  people,  or  events  through  observing 
others  undergoing  emotional  experiences.  However,  the  findings  reveal 
wide  individual  differences  in  the  degree  to  which  people  aic  affected  bj 
the  emotional  aiousal  of  others.  Bandura  and  Rosenthal  reasoned  Iliitl 
observers  who  are  easily  susceptible  to  emotional  reactions  and  who  ;irc 
emotionally  aroused  at  the  time  of  exposure  to  the  affective  expressions 
of  others  will  show  the  strongest  emotional  learning.  To  tost  UK 
hypotheses,  adults  observed  a  model  performing  a  task  when  a  buv./ei 
sounds  and  then  the  model  feigns  an  expression  of  pain  as  though  he  Imd 
been  shocked.  Throughout  this  period,  the  observers'  physiological 
responses  weie  measured  to  determine  the  degree  to  which  the  formerly 
neutral  buzzer  had  taken  on  negative  emotional  value  for  the  observmas 
a  result  of  the  other  person's  adverse  experiences. 

Prior  to  the  emotional  conditioning  phase  of  the  study,  observers  ex- 
perienced different  degrees  of  emotional  arousal  produced  both  phys- 
iologically and  psychologically  through  the  administration  of  epinoplirine, 
a  sympathetic  stimulant.  Before  the  study  began  all  subjects  comnli'K'tl 
the  Taylor  Manifest  Anxiety  Scale  to  provide  a  measure  of  their  genenil 
susceptibility  to  emotional  arousal. 

The  results  show  that  the  observer's  level  of  emotional  arousui  is  n 
significant  determinant  of  vicarious  emotional  learning.  Observers  who 
experienced  either  very  low  or  very  high  arousal  displayed  the  weakest 
vicarious  learning  of  emotional  responses,  whereas  those  who  were  under 
moderate  arousal  were  affected  most.  Interviews  conducted  with  the 
adults  after  the  experiment  was  completed  disclosed  that  those  in  the  hijih 
arousal  groups  neutralized  the  emotion-arousing  situation  by  diverting  I  heir 
attention  from  the  model's  distressing  cues  and  by  conjuring  up  posllive 
or  relaxing  thoughts.  Further  study  of  these  cognitive  activities  may  throw 
light  on  how  people  insulate  themselves  against  the  distressing  experiences 
of  others.  In  this  experiment  some  subjects  felt  extremely  cmpathic  wilh 
the  model  and  others  derived  considerable  satisfaction  from  witnessing 
pain  being  inflicted  upon  the  model. 

Therapeutic  Applications  of  Modeling  Procedures 

Research  in  this  area  has  shown  the  potential  of  modeling  influences  for 
changing  people's  attitudes,  behavior,  and  even  their  personal  standards  of 

258 


self-evaluation.  It  is  also  a  potent  means  of  treating  powerfully  charged 
patterns  such  as  phobias  and  fears  of  long  standing.  One  experiment, 
reported  in  1967  by  Bandura,  Grusec,  and  Menlove,  treated  children  who 
were  extremely  afraid  of  dogs.  During  the  couise  of  treatment,  the  phobic 
children  who  observed  a  bold  peer-model  handle  dogs  comfortably  and 
appropriately,  lost  their  fears. 

After  being  referred  by  their  parents,  the  children  weie  given  stand- 
ardized perfoimance  tests,  on  the  basis  of  which  24  boys  and  24  girls, 
ranging  in  ages  from  3  to  5  years,  were  selected. 

The  initial  selection  test  included  a  graded  sequence  of  tasks  which 
involved  increasingly  intimate  interactions  with  a  dog.  Initially,  the 
investigators  brought  the  children  into  a  room  where  a  cocker  spaniel  was 
confined  in  a  modified  playpen.  Later,  tasks  were  required  which  tanged 
from  walking  up  to  and  looking  at  the  dog,  to  finally  climbing  into  the 
pen  with  the  dog,  petting  her,  scratching  her,  and  then  remaining  alone 
with  the  dog  in  the  room. 

The  children  were  then  assigned  to  one  of  the  following  treatments;  a 
modeling-positive-context  where  a  fearless  peer  model  exhibited  progres- 
sively bold  interaction  with  a  dog  in  the  midst  of  an  enjoyable  partylike 
atmosphere;  a  modeling-neutral-context  where  the  subjects  observed  the 
same  type  of  brave  behavior  modeled,  but  in  a  neutral  atmosphere;  an 
exposure-no-rnodel  condition  (here  the  children  saw  the  dog,  but  with  the 
model  absent);  a  positive-context  group  which  participated  in  the  party, 
but  were  never  exposed  to  either  the  dog  or  the  model. 

The  day  after  the  treatment  series  was  completed  the  children  were 
tested  with  the  experimental  animal;  then  about  IVa  hours  later,  with  an 
unfamiliar  white  mongrel.  The  dogs  had  been  tested  prior  to  the 
experiment,  and  it  was  established  that  they  were  about  the  same  in  terms 
of  activity  level  and  attractiveness.  Half  the  children  were  tested  with  the 
familiar  animal  first,  and  then  with  the  unfamiliar  white  mongrel;  for  the 
remainder,  the  sequence  was  reversed. 

A  month  later  a  followup  evaluation  was  done,  and  the  children  were 
again  tested  to  determine  the  stability  of  the  treatment  effects,  as 
manifested  by  the  children's  willingness  to  interact  fearlessly  with  the 
dogs.  The  two  groups  of  children  who  observed  the  peer  model  interact 
fearlessly  with  the  dog,  achieved  and  retained  substantial  reduction  in 
their  fears  of  dogs.  In  an  effort  to  minimize  the  cognitive  aspects,  all  the 
children  were  Informed  that  the  test  animals  were  harmless.  After  the 
experiment  was  over,  the  children  were  told  that  while  most  dogs  were 
friendly,  before  petting  an  unfamiliar  dog  they  should  ask  the  owner.  This 
was  done  to  reduce  indiscriminate  acts  by  the  children  toward  strange 
dogs, 

The  effect  of  the  modeling  was  obvious;  the  atmosphere,  whether 
partylike  or  not,  had  a  minor  effect,  if  any. 

The  investigators  speculate  that  several  factors  are  involved  in  the 
extinction  or  disappearance  of  the  avoidant  behavior.  One  is  simply  that 
as  the  child  acquires  more  information  about  dogs  and  about  contact  with 
dogs,  he  becomes  less  fearful.  The  nonoccurrence  of  anticipated  adverse 

259 


consequences  to  the  model,  plus  the  pleasure  the  model  has  from  contocl 
with  the  clog,  may  help  extinguish  the  fear  leaction  as  well. 

Treatment  of  Dog  Phobia  Through  Filmed  Modeling 

Another  experiment  was  done  (Bandura  and  Menlove,  1968)  with 
children  who  were  seriously  fearful  of  dogs.  One  group  was  shown  a  movie 
which  demonstrated  how  a  single  model  would  display  progressively  less 
fearful  interactions  with  a  dog,  as  in  the  pieceding  experiment.  Another 
group  observed  a  movie  which  showed  boys  and  girls  of  varying  ages 
interacting  positively  with  a  variety  of  dogs  of  different  sizes  and  dis- 
positions. Children  in  the  control  group  were  shown  movies  with  no 
canine  characteis. 

Results  showed  that  children  who  observed  approach  behavior  which 
resulted  in  no  adverse  effects  to  the  model,  displayed  enduring  and 
generalized  reductions  in  their  own  concerns  about  dogs.  Controls  showed 
no  change.  Comparison  of  the  final  step  achieved  (i.e.,  staying  with  a  dog 
alone)  by  children  who  had  seen  the  single  model  and  those  who  had  seen 
the  movie  with  many  models  showed  that  the  latter  approach  was 
superior.  However,  although  modeling  was  equally  effective  regardless  of 
the  severity  of  the  children's  phobic  behavior,  those  who  manifested  a 
wide  variety  of  fears  benefited  somewhat  less  from  the  multiple  modeling 
technique  than  cliildren  who  had  fewer  fears. 

The  control  children  were  shown  the  multiple  model  film  after  the 
main  experiment  was  completed.  They  were  markedly  more  able  to 
handie  dogs  after  this. 

The  investigators  found  here  that  the  symbolic  portrayal  is  less 
powerful  than  live  demonstrations.  A  single  model  seen  live  is  more 
effective  than  a  single  model  shown  in  a  movie.  However,  a  movie  can  be 
made  more  powerful  by  using  multiple  models  and  a  wider  variety  of 
objects  than  it  is  usually  practical  to  provide  live. 

Snake  Phobia  Project 

This  project,  conducted  by  Bandura,  Blanchard,  and  Ritter,  was  carried 
out  with  adolescent  and  adult  subjects  who  were  terrified  of  snakes.  In  the 
area  of  California  where  Stanford  is  located,  snakes  are  prevalent  enough 
to  seriously  limit  the  life  choices  of  any  adult  who  is  severely  frightened 
by  snakes.  For  instance,  it  would  mean  he  couldn't  be  in  any  job  where  he 
would  be  required  to  inspect  houses,  read  meters,  show  real  estate,  do 
plumbing,  or  any  activity  where  he  might  be  out  of  doors  or  in  basements, 
He  would  be  limited  as  to  the  location  of  his  home  and  be  largely  unable 
to  participate  in  many  popular  local  sports,  such  as  hiking  or  camping. 

Fear  of  snakes  is  considered  by  psychologists  to  be  a  relatively  stable 
fear  and  for  that  reason  is  often  used  in  laboratory  experiments. 

The  subjects  ranged  in  age  from  14  to  60  years  of  age.  Some  of  the 
phobias  had  existed  for  1 5  to  20  years.  In  the  initial  phase  of  the  experi- 

260 


ment,  the  participants  were  administered  a  behavioral  test  that  measured 
the  strength  of  their  avoidance  of  snakes.  In  addition,  they  completed  a 
comprehensive  fear  inventory.  This  inventory  was  then  available  for  deter- 
mination later  to  determine  if  reduction  in  anxiety  about  snakes  brought 
about  other  changes. 

The  cases  were  individually  matched  on  the  basis  of  their  avoidance 
behavioi  and  assigned  to  one  of  four  conditions.  One  group  participated  in 
a  symbolic  modeling  treatment  where  they  would  run  for  themselves  a 
film  depicting  young  children,  adolescents,  and  adults  engaging  in  progres- 
sively more  threatening  situations  and  interactions  with  a  large  (about 
4-feet  long)  king  snake.  The  subjects  were  taught  to  be  relaxed  during  the 
film.  They  were  told  to  stop  the  film  when  scenes  made  them  anxious, 
reverse  it  to  the  beginning,  and  watch  it  over.  They  were  asked  to  attempt 
to  achieve  deep  relaxation  at  the  same  time.  They  were  to  view  the 
threatening  scene  repeatedly  until  it  was  neutralized  for  them. 

The  second  group,  receiving  live  modeling  with  guided  participation, 
watched  a  model  handle  a  snake  in  increasing  proximity  until  it  was 
wrapped  around  him.  The  subjects  were  then  aided  in  perfoiming  with  the 
snake.  The  model  held  the  snake  and  had  the  subject  touch  it,  stroke  it, 
and  then  gradually  hold  it  until  anxieties  about  contact  were  gone.  Then 
the  subject  and  the  model  performed  the  tasks  together  until  the  clients 
were  able  to  hold  the  snake  in  their  laps,  to  let  it  crawl  around,  and  finally 
to  retrieve  it. 

The  third  group  received  a  form  of  desensitization  treatment.  Deep 
relaxation  was  paired  with  imagined  scenes  of  interactions  with  snakes.  As 
in  other  conditions,  the  treatment  was  continued  until  the  clients' 
anxieties  had  disappeared  or  until  the  maximum  time  of  6  hours  allotted 
had  passed.  This  time  limit  was  imposed  upon  all  groups. 

Subjects  in  the  control  condition  participated  in  the  behavioral  and 
attitudinal  assessments  without  receiving  any  intervening  treatment. 

In  the  assessment  phase,  all  initial  tests  were  readministered.  In  order  to 
test  the  generality  of  extinction  effects,  half  the  clients  in  each  of  the 
conditions  were  tested  with  the  now  familiar  brown-striped  king  snake 
and  then  with  an  unfamiliar  crimson-splotched  corn  snake  that  appeared 
strikingly  different.  The  rest  of  the  groups  saw  the  snakes  in  reverse  order. 

The  subjects  weie  asked  to  look  at,  touch,  and  hold  a  snake  with  bare 
and  gloved  hands;  to  remove  the  snake  from  its  cage,  let  it  loose  in  the 
room,  and  then  replace  it  in  the  cage;  to  hold  it  within  5  inches  of  their 
faces,  and  finally  to  tolerate  the  snake  in  their  laps  while  they  held  their 
hands  passively  at  their  sides.  Before  and  during  these  tests  clients  rated 
the  intensity  of  their  anxiety  on  scales. 

Control  subjects  remained  unchanged  in  their  ability  to  handle  the 
snake.  The  subjects  who  had  symbolic  modeling  and  desensitization  had 
substantial  reductions  in  phobic  behavior,  and  live  modeling  combined 
with  guided  participation  proved  to  be  an  unusually  powerful  treatment 
that  eliminated  snake  phobias  in  virtually  all  subjects  (92  percent).  The 
modeling  procedures  not  only  extinguished  avoidance  responses  of  long 
standing,  but  they  also  neutralized  the  anxiety-arousing  properties  of  the 

261 


phobic  objects.  Both  of  the  modeling  tieatments  achieved  marked 
decrements  in  anticipatoiy  and  peiformance  anxiety.  Although  subjects 
who  had  received  desensitization  treatment  also  experienced  less 
emotional  arousal  when  approaching  a  snake,  the  magnitude  of  theii  fear 
reduction  was  significantly  less  than  that  shown  by  their  counterparts  in 
the  modeling  conditions. 

It  was  found  that  attitude  changes  toward  snakes  occurred.  The  more 
potent  the  treatment  and  the  moie  changed  the  subjects  ability  to  handle 
the  snake,  the  greater  the  positive  change  in  attitude. 

In  addition,  other  fears  were  affected  by  the  removal  of  the  snake 
phobias.  Fear  of  other  issues  was  relieved  in  proportion  to  the  potency  of 
the  treatments  employed.  For  instance,  live  modeling  with  subject 
paiticipation  effected  widespread  fear  reductions,  not  only  related  to 
animal  anxieties,  but  also  in  relation  to  a  variety  of  threats  involving  both 
interpersonal  and  nonsocial  events.  The  investigators  note  that  this  seems 
to  involve  two  different  processes.  The  first  involves  generalization  of 
extinction  effects  from  heated  stimuli  to  related  anxiety  sources.  In  other 
woids,  being  relieved  of  one  serious  fear  makes  a  person  generally  less 
fearful  and  more  able  to  cope  realistically  with  other  concerns.  The 
second  entails  positive  reinforcement  of  a  sense  of  capability.  Having 
successfully  overcome  a  phobia  that  had  plagued  them  for  most  of  their 
lives,  subjects  reported  new  confidence  that  they  could  conquer  other 
problems  and  successfully  deal  with  other  anxiety-arousing  situations. 

A  1-month  followup  assessment  revealed  that  the  beneficial  changes 
produced  in  behavior,  attitudes,  and  emotional  responsiveness  were  ef- 
fectively maintained.  The  clients  also  displayed  evidence  that  the  behavior 
improvements  had  been  canied  over  from  the  therapeutic  to  the  real-life 
situations.  They  were  able  to  hike,  garden,  and  even  help  frightened 
friends  01  childien  overcome  fear  of  snakes. 

It  is  the  conviction  of  these  investigators  that  any  type  of  phobic 
disorder  can  be  successfully  treated  by  this  method  with  considerable 
success.  Subsequent  experiments  show  that  information  alone  docs  not 
contribute  to  therapeutic  change.  It  was  piimarily  through  a  combination 
of  demonstration,  information,  guided  performance,  and  the  control  over 
observational  experiences  that  this  success  was  achieved. 

Other  snake  phobia  treatment  experiments  have  been  done  with 
children  as  subjects,  and  these  have  been  equally  successful, 

A  slightly  different  type  of  therapeutic  program  was  developed  by 
Robert  O'Connor  working  with  Doctor  Bandura  to  improve  the  social 
behavior  of  withdrawn  children  in  a  nursery  school  setting.  A  group  of 
withdrawn  children  was  shown  films  of  children  playing  together  and 
having  a  very  good  time.  Another  group,  as  controls,  was  shown  a  movie 
about  Maiineland,  instead.  The  group  of  withdrawn  children,  who  had 
seen  the  movie  designed  to  help  them  overcome  their  social  inhibitions, 
showed  demonstrable  improvements  in  social  interactions;  those  who  hud 
seen  the  other  film,  showed  no  change  in  their  behavior. 


262 


Conclusions 

There  is  little  doubt  that  filmed  or  televised  images  have  tremendous 
power  to  shape  attitudes  and  behavior.  That  this  deserves  investigation  can 
hardly  be  questioned  when  we  realize  the  almost  universal  contemporary 
exposure  to  TV.  The  complexity  of  the  problem  and  the  successful  study 
of  TV  and  movies  are  best  approached  by  isolating  factor  after  factor,  and 
then  painstakingly  evaluating  the  results.  These  studies  are  even  more 
striking  when  it  is  clear  that  they,  for  the  most  part,  deal  with  the  impact 
of  single  incidents  with  relatively  little  reinforcement,  whereas  the  average 
commercial-viewing  fare  is  repetitive  and  often  highly  glamorized.  The 
multiple  violent  techniques  demonstrated  by  a  wide  variety  of  relatively 
unpunished  people  on  TV  can  be  expected  to  be  highly  effective  in 
teaching,  and  even  in  eliciting  violent  and  aggressive  behavior  in  the 
viewers.  On  the  other  hand,  there  is  the  same  potential  for  influencing 
viewers  toward  positive  action  and  more  constiuctive  methods  of  problem 
solving.  Unfortunately  some  of  the  problem-solving  forums  that  are 
televised,  such  as  the  U.N.  in  critical  debates,  are  repetitious,  monotonous, 
lacking  in  the  pace  and  focused  force  of  the  usual  piogramming,  even 
though  there  is  no  denying  their  importance.  Television  can  contribute  to 
the  dissemination  of  information  and  be  highly  influential  in  developing 
awareness.  However,  this  research  points  out  that,  at  times,  new  informa- 
tion without  some  guide  as  to  action  can  arouse  increased  anxiety.  The 
stream  of  information  about  slums  and  racial  tensions  without 
constructive  proposals  may  illustrate  this  phenomenon. 

Since  people  can  imitate  more  successfully  acts  that  are  within  their 
own  range,  they  may  imitate  the  more  direct  acts  rather  than  the  more 
abstract  ones.  This  is  likely  to  be  especially  true  of  children  who,  for 
instance,  are  more  likely  to  have  extensive  physical  vocabularies  than 
verbal  ones. 

Doctor  Banduia's  research  has  dealt  with  the  simpler  units  of  behavior, 
and  he  indicates  that  he  feels  that  more  research  should  be  done  in  terms 
of  subtle  factors.  Physical  violence  is  not  the  only  kind  of  destructive  act 
and,  perhaps,  even  more  benign  than  some  types  of  interactions  between 
people.  For  example,  the  portrayal  of  racial  prejudice,  the  dramatizing 
and  romanticizing  of  poor  marital  interactions,  lying,  and  cheating  may 
be  more  important  than  the  number  of  fist  fights  and  murders  that  are 
seen.  In  addition,  Dr.  Bandura  states: 

"All  the  laboratory  studies  that  I  have  reported  deal  with  the 
immediate  impact  of  a  single  exposure  to  aggression  on  the  viewer's 
attitude  and  conduct.  While  the  questions  about  immediate  effect 
have  been  clarified  to  some  extent,  we  need  much  more  research  on 
the  cumulative  impact  of  television,  and  the  way  in  which  the 
medium  combines  with  other  beneficial  or  adverse  influences  in  the 
shaping  of  people's  thoughts  and  actions." 

He  also  points  out  that  results  of  recent  studies  of  therapeutic  ap- 
plications of  modeling  show  that  such  influences  can  produce  generalized 
and  enduring  effects. 

263 


Another  factor  is  the  number  of  people  exposed  to  essentially  the  sail 
stimulation.  The  same  visual  images  are  seen  by  people  who  ordinari 
would  not  come  into  contact  with  the  same  influences.  The  cultm 
spread  is  much  larger  than  ever  before  in  history.  Certainly,  an  u 
precedented  audience  all  over  the  world  witnessed  the  Apollo  1 1  walkc 
the  moon. 

This  body  of  research  points  up  the  fallacies  in  several  popular  idea 
One  is  that  violence  only  affects  those  who  are  already  violent  or  deviai 
and  involved  in  aggression.  This  has  not  been  borne  out.  All  viewers  ten 
to  be  affected.  Normal  children  also  learn  and  are  encouraged  to  peifon 
aggressive  acts  by  viewing  them  under  certain  circumstances.  Another  idc 
is  that  if  parents  instill  in  their  children  adequate  standards  of  what  i 
right  or  wrong,  the  violence  they  see  will  "wash  over  them."  It  was  clcarl; 
demonstrated   that  even  where  children  can  label  behavior  as  bad  o 
wrong,  if  it  was  successful,  they  may  imitate  it  and  the  conflicts  would  b> 
resolved  more  often  by  a  revaluation  downward  of  the  worth  or  the  roll 
of  the  victim.  Whether  or  not  the  observed  aggressive  acts  are  successfu 
becomes  more  important  than  the  moral  value  of  these  aggressive  acts. 

Perhaps  the  most  prominent  idea  which  has  been  questioned  is  thatoi 
catharsis.  There  is  no  evidence  that  viewing  violence,  at  least  in  mosl 
forms,  dissipates  aggressive  drives  and  makes  a  person  more  healthy.  In 
fact,  it  has  been  demonstrated  that  a  frustrated  viewer  watching  violence 
would  become  less  inhibited  and  more  likely  to  act  on  violent  impulses. 

The  difference  of  sex  roles  and  the  impact  of  male  and  female  models 
have  been  briefly  discussed  and  seem  important.  Clearly,  our  society 
works  toward  helping  girls  inhibit  aggression  and  to  enhance  masculine 
roles  at  the  same  time.  The  boys  are  given  more  latitude  towards  ag- 
gressive expression  and  less  toward  the  emulation  of  any  feminine  traits. 
Still  the  effects  cannot  be  oversimplified,  as  can  be  seen  in  the  new 
phenomenon  of  unisex  clothes  and  the  girls  who  picket,  swear,  and  attack 
the  police;  and  the  boys  who  embrace  nonviolence  even  to  the  point  of 
choosing  prison  over  the  army.  What  part  in  this  was  played  by  TV  is  not 
fully  understood.  This  is  the  generation,  however,  that  is  often  referred  to 
as  the  "TV  generation"  and  one  of  the  first  groups  to  have  been  exposed 
to  its  influence  during  the  entire  span  of  their  lives.  Doctor  Bnndura 
points  out: 

"It  is  evident  that  observers  do  not  function  as  passive  videotape 
recorders  which  indiscriminately  register  and  store  all  modeling 
stimuli  encountered  in  everyday  life." 

The  tremendous  prices  commanded  by  advertising  time  on  TV  would 
alone  testify  to  the  power  that  both  the  industry  and  the  public  attribute 
to  it. 

Learning  often  takes  place  in  a  neutral  setting  and  even  with  strict 
prohibitions,  and  then  acts  are  later  effectively  performed.  The  police 
recruit  learns  to  shoot  on  a  range,  the  army  enlistee  at  a  camp.  Later  they 
shoot  people,  Undoubtedly  parents  can  have  a  considerable  effect  on  their 
children's  activities,  either  by  monitoring  what  is  seen  or  by  encouraging 

264 


or  discouraging  imitation.  This  research  shows,  too,  that  the  learned 
behavior  may  still  be  retained.  However,  it  will  take  more  to  break  down  a 
parental  prohibition  if  it  is  firmly  expressed  on  the  part  of  the  parent  than 
if  no  intervention  is  attempted. 

Many  issues,  such  as  moral  and  personal  achievement  standards,  often 
considered  the  province  of  the  parent,  school,  and  church  are  now  being 
directly  and  powerfully  influenced  by  other  sources,  such  as  TV. 
Certainly,  censorship  seems  a  limited  answer.  It  is  doubtful,  for  instance, 
that  merely  banishing  cigarette  commercials  from  TV  would  have  been  as 
effective  as  the  antismokmg  campaign  has  been.  It  may  be  that  the  key 
lies  in  the  presentation  of  a  broader  variety  of  ideas  and  more  objective 
information. 


Research  Grant.  MH  5162 
Date  of  Interview:  October  1968 

References' 

Bandura,  A.  Behavioral  modifications  through  modeling  procedures.  In:  Krasner,  L., 

and  Ullmann,  L.  P.,  eds.  Research  in  Behavior  Modification.    New  York:  Holt, 

Rinehart  and  Winston,  1965,  pp.  310-340. 
. Influence   of   models'   reinforcement    contingencies    on   the   acquisition  of 

imitative  responses.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  1  (6).  589-595, 

1965. 

..Vicarious  processes;  a  case  of  no-trial  learning.  In:  Berkowitz.,  L.,  ed.  Advances 


in  Experimental  Social  Psychology.  Vol.  II.  New  York;  Academic  Press,  1965.  pp. 
1-55 

Role  of  modeling  processes  in  personality  development.  The  Young  Child: 


Reviews  of  Research.  Published  by  the  National  Association  for  the  Education  of 
Young  Children,  Washington,  D.C.,  1967. 

A  social  learning  interpretation  of  psychological  dysfunctions.  In:  London,  P., 


and  Rosenhan,  D.,  eds.  Foundations  of  Abnormal  Psychology.  New  York:  Holt, 
Rinehart  and  Winston,  1968,  pp.  293-344. 

.Modelling  approaches  to  the  modification  of  phobic  disorders.  Ciba  Founda- 


tion Symposium.    The  Role  of  Learning  in  Psychotherapy.  London;  Churchill, 
1968,  pp.  201-2J7. 

.Principles  of  Behavior  Modification.  New  York:  Holt,  Rinehart  and  Winston, 


1969. 

.Social-learning  theory  of  identificatory  processes.  In:  Goslin,  D.A.,  ed.  Hand- 


book of  Socialization  Theory  and  Research.  Chicago;  Rand  McNally,  1969.  pp. 

213-262. 
Bandura,  A.;  Blanchard,  E.B.;  and  Ritter,  B.  The  relative  efficacy  of  desensitization 

and  modeling  approaches  for  inducing  behavioral,  affective,  and  attitudinal  changes. 

Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  1969, in  press. 
Bandura,  A.;  Grusec,  J.;  and  Menlove,  F.  Vicarious  extinction  of  avoidance  behavior. 

Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  5(1):  16-23,  1967. 
Some  social  determinants  of  self-monitoring  reinforcement  systems.  Journal  of 

Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  5(4):  449455,  1967, 
Bandura,  A.,  and  Harris,  M.B.  Modification  of  syntactic  style.  Journal  of  Experimental 

Child  Psychology,  4;  341-352,  1966. 
Bandura,  A.,  and  Kupers,  C.  Transmission  of  patterns  of  self-reinforcement  through 

modeling.  Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Social  Psychology,  69(1):  1-9,  1964. 
Bandura,  A.,  and  Menlove,  F.  Factors  determining  vicarious  extinction  of  avoidance 

behavior  through  symbolic  modeling.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology, 

8(2):  99-108,  1968. 

265 


Bandiira,  A  ,  and  McDonald,  F.  Influence  of  social  reinforcement  and  the  behavior  o 

models  in   shaping  children's  moral  judgments.  Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Socia 

Psychology,  67(3)'  274-281,  1963. 
Bandura,  A  ,  and  Mischel,  W.  Modification  of  self-imposed  delay  of  reward  througl 

exposure  to  live  and  symbolic  models.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psy 

chology,  2(5}'  648-705,  1965. 
Bandura,    A.,   and    Perloff,   B.    Relative  efficacy   of  self -monitored   and   external!; 

imposed  reinforcement  systems.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology 

7(2)"  111-116,  1967. 
Bandura>   A.,    and    Rosentlial,   T.L.  Vicarious  classical  conditioning  as  function  o 

arousal  level. /ouMa/  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  3(1):  54-62,  1966. 
Bandura,  A  ;  Ross,  D.;  and  Ross,  S.  Imitation  of  film  mediated  aggressive  models 

Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Social  Psy  chology,  66'  3-11,  1963 
Bandura,  A.;  Ross,  D.,  and  Ross,  S.A.  Vicarious  reinforcement  and  imitative  learning 

Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Social  Psychology,  67:  601-607,  1963. 
Bandura,  A.,  and  VVhalen,  CK.  Influence  of  antecedent  reinforcement  and  divergent 

modeling   cues   on   patterns  of  self-reward.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social 

Psychology,  3(4):  372-382,  1966. 
Hart  ma  nn,  D.P.  Influence  of  symbolically  modeled  instrumental  aggression  and  pain 

cues  on  aggressive  behavior.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  11: 

280-288,  1969. 
O'Connor,  R.D.  Modification  of  social  withdrawal  through  symbolic  modeling.  Journal 

of  Applied  fiehavior  Analysis,  1:  15-22,  1969 


266 


Investigator 

Theodore  Lidz,  M.D. 

Yale  University  School  of  Medicine 

New  Haven,  Conn. 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 

An  intensive  study  of  families  with  a  schizophrenic  offspring  has  led 
Yale  investigators  to  characterize  schizophrenia  as  a  deficiency  disease. 
Schizophrenia,  they  conclude,  results  from  a  deficiency  in  the  nurturing 
supplied  by  the  parents,  plus  a  deficiency  in  the  transmission  from  parents 
to  child  of  the  basic  techniques,  particularly  those  dealing  with  language, 
that  he  needs  for  adapting  himself  to  the  world  when  he  leaves  the  family. 

On  the  basis  of  their  findings,  Drs.  Theodore  Lidz  and  Stephen  Fleck 
suggest  that  schizophrenia  may  be  passed  along  almost  as  truly  as  certain 
physical  characteristics  but  without  any  involvement  of  the  genes.  Dr. 
Lidz  is  professor  of  psychiatry  at  the  Yale  Univeisity  School  of  Medicine 
and  a  career  investigator  of  the  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health;  Dr. 
Fleck  is  professor  of  psychiatry  and  public  health  and  psychiatrist-in-chief 
of  the  Yale  Psychiatric  Institute,  where  the  patients  were  hospitalized. 

Many  authorities  suspect  that  an  inherited  biochemical  abnormality 
plays  at  least  some  role  in  schizophrenia.  The  Yale  investigatois  do  not 
deny  that  this  may  be  so,  but  they  find  no  reason  to  think  that  it  must  be 
so.  They  believe  that  schizophrenia  can  be  fully  explained  on  the  basis  of 
what  happens  to  a  person  within  his  family  during  the  first  two  decades  of 
his  life.  In  general  this  depends  upon  the  adequacy  of  his  mother  and 
father  to  fill  their  roles  as  parents,  and  this  in  turn  is  influenced  strongly 
by  their  experiences  as  children  with  their  own  parents. 

In  every  family  studied,  at  least  one  of  the  parents  was  judged  to  be 
seriously  disturbed.  Many  of  the  other  parents  were  judged  to  be,  if  not 
seriously  disturbed,  rather  difficult  and  peculiar.  Generally  the  problems 
of  the  parents  were  found  to  have  antedated  the  marriage. 

Dr.  Lidz  and  his  associates,  principally  Dr.  Fleck  and  Alice  Cornelison, 
a  research  social  worker,  began  their  investigations  in  1953.  They  wanted 
to  learn  whether  or  not  there  was  something  specific  within  the  family 
circle  that  might  be  responsible  for  the  appearance  of  schizophrenia  in  an 
offspring. 

Their  interest  in  the  family  sprang  from  the  concept  of  schizophrenia  as 
a  condition  in  which  a  person  fails  to  achieve  a  workable  integration  of  his 
personality  by  late  adolescence  or  early  adult  life.  Unable  to  direct 
liimself,  he  then  retreats  into  asocial  ways  of  living.  Characteristically  he 
does  so  by  breaking  with  the  way  the  people  of  his  culture  think  and 

267 


communicate.  His  failure,  the  investigators  reasoned,  might  result  from 
failures  in  the  way  he  had  been  prepared  to  take  up  life  as  a  reasonably 
independent  adult.  Since  the  family  is  the  fundamental  training  place  and 
the  parents  the  most  important  influence  upon  the  developing  child,  an 
intensive  study  of  family  backgrounds  appeared  essential.  Indications 
from  recent  studies  that  schizophrenic  patients  had  grown  up  in  seriously 
disturbed  families  reinforced  the  investigators'  line  of  reasoning. 

The  research  team  selected  only  families  in  which  it  could  study,  \\\ 
addition  to  the  patient,  at  least  the  mother  and  one  brother  ot  sister; 
almost  always  the  father  was  available,  too.  These  and  other  relatives,' 
together  with  teachers  and  friends  of  the  patient,  and  friends  and  as- 
sociates of  the  parents,  then  and  earlier,  were  interviewed  at  length  in  tin 
effort  to  get  an  intimate  and  detailed  family  picture.  No  other  series  of 
families,  the  investigators  believe,  has  ever  been  so  thoroughly  studied. 

Single,  specific  causative  factors  have  not  been  found.  Schizophrenia 
developed  in  the  families  studied,  the  investigates  conclude,  because  the 
parents  had  failed  to  carry  out  the  tasks  essential  to  the  adequate  biinginp 
up  of  children.  The  study  groups  these  tasks  into  three  functions:  (1) 
parental  nurturance,  meaning  normal  love  and  care;  (2)  the  proper 
structuring  of  the  personality,  which  is  achieved  through  a  family  struc- 
ture in  which  the  parents  support  each  other,  carry  out  the  roles  ap- 
propriate to  their  sex,  and  respect  the  boundaries  between  generations;  (3) 
the  transmission  of  the  techniques  essential  for  adaptation  to  the  culture 
in  which  the  developing  human  being  finds  himself.  In  the  families 
studied,  the  investigators  report,  there  was  a  failure  to  carry  oul 
adequately  not  just  one  but  all  three  of  these  functions. 

"There  is  nothing  so  mysterious  about  schizophrenia,"  Dr.  Lid/ 
remarks.  "It  is  only  mysterious  when  you  go  to  the  back  wards  of  ;i 
hospital  and  see  the  people  who  have  been  there  for  many  years  why  arc 
just  standing  rigidly  or  jabbering  to  themselves.  But  if  you  watch  young 
people  coming  into  the  hospital  and  take  your  time  to  get  their  slory,  il 
makes  sense.  If  a  person  really  understood  human  development,  in  certain 
families  he  would  have  to  start  looking  for  something  like  schizophrenia 
even  though  he  had  never  heard  of  that  condition." 

The  findings  of  the  present  study,  the  researchers  believe,  provide  Ideas 
both  for  preventing  and  treating  schizophrenia.  There  is  some  indication 
that  they  may  also  lead  to  a  means  of  predicting  in  which  families 
schizophrenia  is  likely  to  develop. 

The  sample  investigated  is  small,  17  families,  so  the  researchers  point 
out  that  validating  studies  are  essential.  It  is  also  unrepresentative,  since 
the  families  could  afford  care-costing  thousands  of  dollars  a  year  in  ;in 
outstanding  private  mental  hospital.  All  the  families  except  two  wm 
rated  either  upper  class  or  upper  middle  class,  although  schizophrenia  Es 
more  prevalent  at  the  lowest  socioeconomic  levels.  The  researchers  explain 
that  they  wanted  to  avoid  the  complexities  created  by  economic  distress 
and  related  conditions  as  factors  contributing  to  whatever  family  defici- 
encies might  be  found.  As  it  happened,  the  series  differed  from  the  cases 

268 


usually  studied  in  that  most  of  the  families  were  intact,  but  this  circum- 
stance, too,  the  investigators  think,  helped  them  clarify  the  conditions 
essential  for  the  development  of  this  most  common  of  mental  diseases. 

The  researchers  believe  that  the  findings  based  on  the  sample  studied 
will  stand  up.  "We've  had  at  least  200  schizophrenic  patients  at  the  Yale 
Psychiatric  Institute  since  the  study  began,"  Dr.  Fleck  repoits,  "and 
everything  we  know  about  those  200  fits  in  with  what  we've  learned  in 
our  intensive  study  of  the  17." 

A  recent  French  study  of  primarily  lower  class  families  is  reported  to 
supply  at  least  some  evidence  that  the  Yale  team's  findings  are  applicable 
to  families  at  all  levels. 

When  the  patients  were  taken  into  the  study,  they  were  in  their  teens  or 
twenties.  Some  had  only  recently  broken  down;  others  had  been  sick  a 
long  time  and  had  been  treated  in  other  institutions.  They  and  their 
families  were  studied  over  periods  ranging  from  4  months  to  10  years.  In 
some  cases  the  number  of  factfinding  interviews— in  addition  to  the 
therapeutic  interviews  with  the  patient  and,  sometimes,  other  members  of 
the  family -ran  into  the  hundreds. 

Members  of  the  research  team  have  been  publishing  their  findings  in 
professional  journals  as  the  work  has  progressed.  The  present  report  is 
written  as  the  main  study  neais  its  end.  It  summarizes  the  principal 
findings,  notes  some  of  the  research  problems  encountered,  and  discusses 
work  that  has  grown  out  of  the  investigation. 


Troubled  Marriages;  Two  General  Types 

In  more  than  half  the  families  with  a  schizophienic  offspring,  the 
research  team  reports,  the  parents'  problems  had  led  to  marital  schism. 
This  is  defined  as  a  state  of  seveie  chronic  disequilibrium  and  discord, 
which  aggravated  the  personality  troubles  of  each  parent  and  constantly 
threatened  the  marriage.  In  the  other  cases  the  parents'  problems  had  led 
to  marital  skew,  a  state  in  which,  though  the  marriage  was  not  constantly 
threatened,  the  more  normal  parent  allowed  the  psychopathology  of  the 
other  one  to  dominate  the  home  and  thus  distort  the  child's  development. 

As  an  example  of  schismatic  marriage,  the  investigators  discuss  a  couple 
they  call  Mr.  and  Mrs.  Grau,1  the  wife  Catholic,  the  husband  bitterly 
anti-Catholic.  Soon  after  marriage,  Mr.  Grau  informed  his  wife  that  the 
children's  religion  would  never  cause  trouble  because  there  weren't  going 
to  be  any  children.  When,  nevertheless,  Nancy  was  born  a  few  years  later, 
he  refused -to  his  wife's  distress-to  let  her  be  baptized. 

Looking  back  during  her  late  teens,  after  schizophrenia  had  set  in,  this 
girl  could  remember  no  period  when  her  parents  had  not  been  fighting 
openly  and  threatening  to  separate.  Her  recollections  corresponded  with 
those  of  her  younger  sister,  Ellen.  At  issue  besides  religion  had  been  Mrs, 
Grau's  child-rearing  practices  (she  was  overly  protective  of  Nancy  as  a 


1  All  names  are  fictitious. 

269 


young  child  and  then,  as  the  girl  grew  up,  highly  intrusive),  the  propei 
amount  of  formal  education  (at  least  4  years  of  college,  asserted  Mr.  Gran, 
deriding  his  wife  because  she  had  only  two),  and  a  number  of  othei 
matters.  The  investigators  describe  the  husband  as  chronically  irritable  and 
paranoidly  suspicious.  Although  his  wife  had  been  unusually  insecure 
about  motherhood,  she  might  have  functioned  more  adequately,  they 
suggest,  had  she  received  at  least  a  reasonable  amount  of  emotional 
suppoit  from  her  husband. 

Mr.  and  Mrs.  Newcomb  are  presented  as  one  example  of  the  other 
general  type  of  marriage-skewed.  Even  though  Mrs.  Newcomb  was 
exceedingly  difficult,  she  and  her  husband  got  along  well  because  he  was 
constantly  grateful  for  her  attention  and  deferrent  to  her  judgment. 

When  the  first  child,  Jack,  was  born,  the  father  bathed  him  because  the 
mother  feared  she  might  accidentally  drown  him.  Jack  and  a  younger 
sister  were  virtually  isolated  from  other  children  until  they  went  to 
school.  Then  Mrs.  Newcomb  pestered  the  teachers  with  demands  that  the 
children's  special  abilities  be  recognized.  When  Jack  and  his  sister 
quarreled  with  their  mother  in  later  years,  Mr.  Newcomb 's  only  advice  was 
to  do  as  he  did  and  never  oppose  her.  Jack  became  psychotic  during  his 
freshman  year  at  college. 

Whether  skewed  or  schismatic,  all  the  marriages  studied  had  the  same 
general  effect:  the  production  of  an  environment  so  deleterious  to  normal 
development  that  a  person  need  not  look  beyond  it,  the  investigatois 
believe,  to  explain  why  at  least  one  child  in  each  family  became  schizo- 
phrenic. 


Faults  in  Family  Structure 

The  research  group  has  drawn  out  the  factors  considered  common  to 
each  maniage  and  of  significance  in  producing  the  harmful  environment. 
Some  of  these  factors  are  viewed  as  deficiencies  in  the  organization  or 
structure  of  the  family;  the  others,  as  disturbances  in  the  way  the  parents, 
largely  unconsciously,  conveyed  the  essential  techniques  for  adaptation. 

In  matters  of  family  structure  the  following  deficiencies  were  found: 
I.  The  Parents  Failed  To  Form  a  Coalition 

husbands  and  wives  have  differing  roles  and  functions,  the 
-joint  out,  these  should  interrelate  to  form  a  unit  in  regard  to 


When  a  marriage  is  working  properly,  each  parent  supports  the  other's 
role,  thus  providing  some  of  the  assurance  and  strength  of  other  must 
have  to  perform  it.  Further,  as  the  result  of  this  mutual  support,  the 
investigators  explain,  the  child's  natural  tendency  to  divide  the  parents 
and,  in  fantasy,  shove  one  out  in  order  to  have  the  other  for  himself-the 
Oedipus  complex  of  psychoanalytic  theory -is  naturally  frustrated,  so  the 
child's  development  proceeds  in  the  normal  direction.  He  grows  up  to 
value  maniage  and  to  see  it  as  a  union  in  which  each  person  works  for  the 
other's  satisfaction  as  well  as  his  own. 

This  structural  necessity,  parental  coalition,  may  often  become 
weakened  in  normal  families,  the  investigators  assert,  but  in  the  families 
investigated  it  had  either  collapsed  or  become  ciitically  distorted  very 
early,  if  indeed  it  had  ever  been  achieved. 

In  the  schismatic  marriages,  husbands  and  wives  criticized  and  devalued 
each  other,  thus  placing  an  almost  unbearable  burden  upon  the  children; 
for  in  the  natural  course  of  development,  Dr.  Lidz  explains,  a  child 
identifies  himself  with,  and  models  himself  upon,  the  parent  of  the  same 
sex.  The  other  parent  serves  as  a  model,  too—  of  the  kind  of  person  with 
whom  the  child  will  seek  to  unite  when  he  leaves  the  family.  (The 
transformation  of  the  person  he  selects  into  the  kind  of  person  he  has 
been  unwittingly  seeking,  Dr.  Lidz  notes,  is  more  readily  achieved  "when 
his  perception  is  blurred  by  sexual  impulsion.") 

But  it  is  terribly  difficult  for  a  boy  to  accept  his  father  as  a  model-in 
spite  of  the  very  strong  natural  tendency  to  do  so-if  the  father  is 
constantly  being  devalued  by  the  mother,  whose  love  the  boy  seeks, 
Similar  difficulty  and  confusion  arise  when  a  girl  hears  her  mother 
criticized  and  disparaged.  The  trouble  is  compounded  because  the  parent 
who  normally  is  a  primary  or  basic  love  object,  or  desired  source  of 
affection,  is  constantly  tearing  down  the  parent  with  whom  the  child 
identifies.2 

Even  in  those  families  having  little  overt  conflict  between  husband  and 
wife,  the  study  finds  a  marked  failure  of  the  parents  to  support  each 
other.  Mrs.  Newcomb,  for  example,  did  not  belittle  her  husband  to  his 
face,  but  she  made  clear  her  expectation  that  Jack  would  have  a  brilliant 
career  in  art  rather  than,  like  his  father,  a  moderately  successful  one  in 
business.  Beyond  this,  she  ran  the  family.  Though  the  father  made  the 
money,  he  had  abdicated  the  other  obligations  of  his  parental  role. 

//.    The  Parents  Failed  to  Maintain  the  Essential  Boundaries  Between 

Generations 

Generally  the  failure  to  maintain  the  boundary  between  generations 
was  marked  by  the  effort  of  one  parent -and  sometimes  both-to  satisfy 
through  the  child  an  emotional  need  not  being  met  by  the  other  parent.  In 
the  Nussbaum  family,  following  a  bitter  and  protracted  quarrel,  the  wife 


2  Licte,  T.  The  Family  and  Human  Adaptation.  New  York:  International  Universities 
Press,  1963. 

271 


held  herself  aloof  from  her  husband,  who  then  became  excessively  at- 
tentive to  their  daughter.  He  would  cuddle  her  until  she  went  to  sleep,  and 
would  even  sleep  with  her  on  nights  when  she  woke  up  and  was  afraid. 
This  near-incestuous  lelationship,  as  the  investigators  describe  it,  was 
broken  off  by  the  girl  late  in  adolescence  in  sudden  terror  that  she  was 
pregnant.  A  little  later  she  became  openly  schizophrenic. 

Seveial  of  the  mothers  said  they  lived  only  for  their  children.  They 
lived  through  them  as  well,  the  study  finds,  unable  to  differentiate  clearly 
between  their  own  needs  and  anxieties  and  those  of  their  offspring.  Such 
mothers  closely  supervised  the  children's  activities,  fought  or  sought  to 
fight  all  their  battles,  were  constantly  intruding.  Some  mothers  behaved  as 
though  their  children  were  little  more  than  extensions  of  themselves, 
living-as  at  birth-in  a  symbiotic  relationship.  One  mother,  for  example, 
when  she  needed  a  laxative,  gave  her  twin  sons  a  laxative  also;  when  the 
doctor  prescribed  a  sedative  for  her,  she  gave  it  to  the  boys  as  well. 

Sometimes  one  parent  competed  with  the  child  for  the  other  parent's 
love  and  attention.  In  the  Lamb  family,  for  example,  the  husband, 
resenting  from  the  start  his  wife's  efforts  to  care  for  their  son,  acted  less 
like  the  boy's  father  than  a  jealous  older  brother.  Later  he  boasted  of  his 
athletic  record  and  appeared  to  want  his  son  to  be  an  athlete,  too.  But  he 
criticized  the  boy's  efforts  and  lost  his  temper  over  them.  Such  rivalry,  Dr. 
Udz  comments,  leads  a  boy  to  stop  trying  to  acquire  masculine  assets  lest 
they  arouse  the  father's  hostility.  At  the  same  time  Mr.  Lamb  belittled  his 
son's  interests  and  achievements  in  other  fields.  If  anyone  was  going  to  be 
encouraged  and  admired,  it  had  to  be  the  father. 

When  the  generation  boundaries  are  confused,  the  researchers  note,  the 
child's  place  in  the  family  is  disturbed,  and  energies  that  should  be  going 
into  furthering  his  own  development  are  drained  off  to  provide  emotional 
support  to  a  parent,  or  to  struggle  with  a  rivalry  imposed  by  a  parent,  or 
merely  to  survive.  His  emotional  development  is  warped ;  he  has  difficulty 
gaining  his  own  identity. 

III.  The  Parents  Failed  to  Maintain  the  Sexual  Roles 
Appropriate  to  Them 

The  sex  of  a  child,  the  investigators  assert,  is  the  most  decisive  factor 
entering  into  the  formation  of  the  child's -and  adult's-personality  charac- 
teristics. Confusions  and  dissatisfactions  concerning  sexual  identity  can 
lead  not  only  to  perversions  but  also  to  neuroses  and  character  defects.3 
In  the  patients  studied  such  confusions  were  also  found  to  be  part  of  the 
complex  of  problems  leading  to  schizophrenia. 

A  person  takes  on  the  attributes  appropriate  to  his  sex,  Dr.  Lidz  points 
out,  not  simply  by  being  born  a  member  of  that  sex  but  by  being  con- 
fronted with  the  appropriate  expectations  from  infancy  onward  nncl  by 
identifying  himself  with  the  appropriate  parent. 


3Lidz,  T.  The  family  and  Human  Adaptation,  Cited  earlier. 
272 


If  the  parent  of  the  same  sex  as  the  child  plays  an  inappropriate  role, 
the  child's  development  is  likely  to  be  warped.  In  some  of  the  families 
studied,  the  role  reversals  were  obvious.  One  husband  mothered  the 
children  and  took  care  of  the  house  while  his  wife  ran  a  business.  Another 
husband  went  to  his  law  office  every  day,  but  it  was  his  wife  who  actually 
earned  his  office  rent  and  the  money  for  all  the  other  bills.  Naturally,  say 
the  investigatois,  the  children  in  these  families  grew  up  with  distorted 
views  of  masculinity  and  femininity. 

In  most  cases  the  failure  to  maintain  the  appropriate  sex-linked  roles  is 
reported  to  have  been  less  obvious  but  just  as  real.  The  psychiatrists 
explain  that  a  girl  needs  to  grow  up  in  the  company  of  a  warm,  expressive, 
helpful  woman  if  she  is  to  have  the  childhood  experiences  that  will  enable 
her  to  fit  easily  into  a  woman's  role  herself,  but  the  mothers  of  the 
schizophrenic  girls  in  this  study  were  found  to  be  distant  and  cool,  toward 
their  daughters  at  least.  A  boy,  if  he  is  to  fit  readily  into  a  man's  role 
himself,  must  grow  up  in  the  company  of  a  man  strong  enough  to 
represent  his  family  to  the  outside  world,  to  live  without  being  over- 
whelmed, and  to  let  his  family  feel  his  love.  But  the  fathers  of  the 
schizophrenic  boys  in  this  study  tended  to  be  either  weak  and  ineffective 
as  husbands  and  parents  (though  usually  successful  as  moneymakers)  or 
else  aloof. 

The  failure  of  a  parent  to  maintain  an  appropriate  role  can  usually  be 
traced  to  that  parent's  personality  problems,  the  research  team  reports, 
but  these  problems  are  often  aggravated  by  the  other  parent.  As  an 
example,  Mr.  Forel  married  one  of  three  sisters  who  were  openly 
contemptuous  of  men.  (His  wife  boasted  to  her  sons  that  as  a  teenager  she 
had  dated  boys  mainly  for  the  pleasure  of  standing  them  up.)  Mrs.  Forel 
laughed  at  her  husband's  efforts  to  make  decisions  for  the  family;  refused 
for  years— until  threatened  with  divorce  and  a  reduced  income—to  move  to 
a  city  where  his  career  would  be  advanced  but  where  she  would  be  a  long 
way  from  her  sisters;  teased  him  sexually  but  denied  him  satisfaction.  Of 
the  two  sons,  the  older  grew  up  trying  to  please  his  mother  and  aunts,  and 
women  in  general,  by  his  effeminate  interests;  the  other  grew  up  clinging 
to  his  father  and  fearing  all  women.  It  was  the  younger  child  who  became 
schizophrenic. 

In  this  case,  the  investigators  suggest,  Mr.  Forel  may  have  been  suf- 
ficiently weak  and  masochistic  to  bring  trouble  upon  himself  and  his 
children  no  matter  whom  he  married,  but  the  trouble  would  have  been 
less  severe  had  he  married  a  more  nearly  normal  woman. 


Defective  Transmission  of  Cultural  Techniques 

Ordinarily  the  family  provides  the  child  not  only  with  the  models  to 
follow  but  also  with  tlie  fundamental  skills  and  techniques  necessary  to 
live  as  an  independent  human  being.  The  most  important  of  these  are  the 
techniques  for  communicating  with  others,  mainly  through  the  use  of 
language.  The  investigators  point  out  that  a  person  must  acquire  the 

273 


language  of  his  culture  in  order  (a)  to  think,  (b)  to  acquire  most  of  the 
other  techniques,  and  (c)  to  associate  constructively  with  other  people. 

In  the  process  of  thinking,  the  researchers  explain,  an  individual  uses 
words  to  build  a  symbolic  veision  of  the  world,  which  he  then  manipulates 
in  imaginative  trial  and  error  in  order  to  arrive  at  the  most  desirable  course 
of  action.  The  extent  to  which  his  representation  of  the  woild  conforms 
with  reality  depends  importantly  upon  the  meanings  of  the  verbal  symbols 
with  which  he  builds  it.  Hence  his  thoughts  and  actions  are  deeply 
influenced  by  the  language  he  learns. 

The  schizophrenic  patients  in  the  Yale  study  had  all  learned  English, 
but  they  had  learned  it  in  environments  that  provided,  in  the  investigators' 
words,  training  in  inationality.  Some  of  the  paients  were  delusional.  A 
man  who  had  made  a  fortune  in  business  spent  most  of  his  spare  time 
isolated  in  his  bedroom  reading  stock  market  reports  and  Eastern 
theology.  He,  his  wife,  and  the  governess  all  believed  him  to  be  the  ic- 
incarnation  of  an  Asiatic  god,  and  they  bi ought  up  the  children  in  the 
same  belief.  A  mother,  writing  in  her  diary  after  her  son  was  born, 
confided  hei  hope  that  she  had  given  birth  to  the  Messiah  and  for  years 
recoided  the  family's  unhappy  life  in  idealized  terms.  Another  mother 
believed  her  telephone  was  tapped.  Mr.  Gran  preached  a  world  conspiracy 

of  Catholics. 

In  most  cases,  however,  the  parents  were  simply  distorting  reality,  un- 
consciously, to  meet  their  own  emotional  needs.  The  Lerners  pretended  to 
the  world  at  large  that  Mr.  Lerncr  was  a  busy  and  respected  lawyer, 
although  in  fact  virtually  all  of  his  clients  had  fallen  away  after  his 
partner's  death  some  years  before.  A  fathei  wrote  each  week  from  a 
distant  city  that  he  would  soon  come  home.  He  never  appeared,  but  his 
wife  kept  assuring  the  children  for  years  that  daddy  would  be  there  "next 
week".  Another  mother  kept  promising  lovely  trips,  but  did  not  keep  her 
promises. 

A  young  patient  remarked  during  the  couise  of  a  family  therapy  session 
that  her  recent  visit  home  had  been  marred  by  her  father's  nagging.  "Your 
father  never  nags,"  the  mother  snapped,  though  ho  had  been  nagging  the 
daughter  just  a  few  minutes  before.  Later  on  the  patient  turned  lo  her 
mother  and  said,  "I  find  that  I'm  often  uneasy  with  you."  "If  you  arc," 
the  mother  replied,  "you're  the  only  person  who  is."  Yet  this  mother 
consistently  upset  almost  everyone  she  talked  to  at  the  hospital. 

Under  such  conditions,  the  research  team  notes,  much  of  the  com- 
munication within  a  family  must  be  unreal,  with  the  result  that  children 


even  more  unmindful  of  messages  the  child  did  not  put  into  words.  The 
impervious  parent  is  not  rejecting  the  child,  the  investigators  believe; 
ratlier,  he  is  rejecting  anything  that  threatens  his  own  equilibrium.  "We 
feel  that  many  of  the  parents  have  a  very  narrow  base  of  stability,  that 
they  are  sort  of  hanging  on-limiting  their  environment  so  that  they  can 
cope  with  it,"  the  study  reports.  By  so  doing,  the  parents  also  limit  what 
the  child  is  able  to  see,  perceive,  or  do,  and  they  create  in  him  despair 
about  the  validity  of  communication. 

The  conclusion  that  the  patients  had  been  trained  in  irrationality  is 
based  not  only  on  the  researchers'  own  expeiience  with  the  parents  and 
on  the  recollections  of  other  members  of  the  family  but  also  on  the  results 
of  two  projective  tests-the  Rorschach,  in  which  a  person  tells  what  he 
sees  in  a  series  of  ink  blots,  and  the  Thematic  Apperception  test,  in  which 
he  tells  what  he  sees  in  a  series  of  pictures. 

Dr.  Margaret  Singer,  a  psychologist  who  made  a  blind  analysis  of  the 
parents1  responses— made  it,  that  is,  without  knowing  anything  about  the 
cases— almost  invariably  and  in  consideiable  detail  confirmed  the  picture 
that  had  been  built  up  by  direct  observation  and  history  taking.  As  an 
example,  in  interpreting  Mrs.  Newcomb's  responses  in  the  Rorschach  test, 
Dr,  Singer  wrote;  "She  takes  a  negativistic  viewpoint  and  kills  off 
meanings  by  saying  that  she  feels  nothing.  She  keeps  conveying  that 
meaning  is  hardly  worth  seeking  because  one  cannot  find  anything 
likeable  or  clear,  and  furthermore,  she  will  not  try.  She  will  not  talk 
directly  about  anything  ...  At  the  same  time  that  she  blurs  meaning- 
fulness,  she  creates  an  aura  of  being  a  nice,  sweet  person  ..." 

After  reading  Mrs.  Newcomb's  responses  to  the  Thematic  Apperception 
Test,  the  psychologist  reported:  "She  is  agonizingly  contradictory,  and 
when  the  tester  inquires  about  an  inconsistency,  Mrs.  Newcomb  simply 
slaps  down  the  examiner  and  further  blurs  meaning  by  stating  that  the 
picture  does  not  make  any  sense  .  .  .Nothing  about  reality  seems  to  please 
her  or  seems  right,  logical,  or  consistent.  Sexuality  is  among  her  worst 
topics.  People  are  both  male  and  female  at  the  same  time." 

The  investigators  report  a  tendency  to  minimize  the  extent  to  which 
children  are  exposed  by  their  parents  to  serious  distortions  of  meanings. 
"Parents  who  are  borderline  schizophrenic  or  somewhat  paranoid,"  the 
study  says,  "are  not  counted  in  the  statistics  of  mental  illness.  A  vague 
and  rambling  mother  who  is  more  or  less  schizophrenic  may  obscure 
meanings  to  an  extent  that  even  a  psychiatrist  has  difficulty  in  com- 
munication with  her,  but  her  children  have  been  exposed  to  her  blurrings 
and  inconsistencies  of  meanings  since  they  were  born.  A  father  who  is 
only  considered  somewhat  rigid  and  overbearing  by  his  business 
colleagues,  at  home  dominates  the  behavior  and  thinking  of  the  family 
with  his  paranoid  rigidity  and  distrust.  Such  circumstances  are  apt  to  be 
more  malignant  when  the  deviances  are  not  sufficiently  pronounced  to  be 
categorized  as  'crazy,'  or  when  the  distortions  of  the  disturbed  parent  are 
accepted  by  the  other  parent,  or  when  the  parent  holds  a  place  of  esteem 
in  the  community  and  therefore  must  be  right." 

275 


The  investigators  report  that  some  of  the  parents  also  failed  to  transmit 
basic  nonlinguistic  skills.  One  patient,  an  artistically  gifted  young  woman, 
for  example,  had  never  learned  how  to  buy  and  adjust  a  brassiere,  or  even 
now  to  put  on  her  stockings  properly.  She  had  never  done  any  cooking, 
and  she  had  never  shopped  in  a  grocery  store. 

Skills,  customs,  and  social  amenities  are  picked  up  not  only  from 
parents  but  also  from  peers.  The  young  people  who  developed  schizo- 
phrenia in  the  families  studied,  however,  tended  to  have  associated  less 
than  usual  with  other  children.  Thus,  the  investigators  point  out,  thcie 
had  been  less  chance  for  eccentricities  learned  at  home  to  be  corrected. 
Since  these  eccentricities  presumably  had  helped  keep  the  child  apart 
from  his  peers,  a  vicious  circle  had  operated.  Most  of  the  patients  became 
overtly  psychotic,  the  study  emphasizes,  only  after  leaving  theii  restricted 
world,  bumping  into  customs  and  ideas  that  seemed  strange  and  becoming 
aware  of  a  confusion -sometimes  terrifying-about  their  roles  as  men  or 
women.  Universities  with  a  good  psychiatric  staff,  Dr.  Lidz  remarks, 
expect  to  find  every  year,  a  few  months  after  the  start  of  school,  a  gioiip 
of  students  requiring  hospitalization.  Detected  and  treated  early,  moslof 
these  can  return  to  college  within  a  year. 


Words,  Meanings,  and  Schizophrenia 

In  schizophrenia,  the  investigators  believe,  the  patient  alters  his  repre- 
sentation of  reality-a  representation  built  with  words— in  order  to  escape 
from  a  world  that  has  grown  untenable.  Faced  with  conflicts  to  which  he 
sees  no  solution,  he  changes  his  perception  of  himself  and  others  ami 
abandons  the  meanings  and  logic  of  his  culture.  He  thus  finds  room  for 
living  and  a  kind  of  self-esteem. 

This  distortion  of  the  thought  processes  without  loss  of  intelligence 
potential,  according  to  the  investigators,  is  what  distinguishes  schi/o- 
phrenia  from  other  types  of  mental  illness.  Generally  the  distortion  occur* 
in  only  certain  areas  of  thought,  "Provide  really  good  care  for  patients," 
says  the  research  team,  "and  they  don't  continue  looking  very  schizo- 
phrenic." 

(One  of  the  patients  in  this  study  learned  analytic  geometry  while  lie 
was  hospitalized;  another  composed  intricate  music;  a  third  analyzed  the 
stock  market  and  selected  a  portfolio  of  stocks  that  would  have  paid  off 
handsomely  had  the  psychiatrist  followed  his  patient's  advice.) 

The  abandonment  of  meanings,  with  the  consequent  distortion  of 
thought,  the  research  group  explains,  isolates  the  patient  and  tends  to 
make  his  condition  self -perpetuating.  He  no  longer  tries  to  match  1m 
concepts  with  those  of  other  people  and  thus  to  learn  whether  or  not  his 
concepts  are  the  ones  required  for  living  normally. 

Why  don't  other  persons  facing  conflicts  that  seem  insoluble  take  Hie 
schizophrenic's  way  out?  Because  they  cannot,  the  investigators  suggest. 
The  schizophrenic  can  take  it  because  he  has  never  attained  a  firm  ami 
useful  system  of  meanings.  More  than  other  people,  he  has  encountered 

276 


serious  difficulties  in  understanding  and  coping  with  the  situations  in 
which  he  finds  himself.  He  has  never  been  sure,  at  least  in  certain  areas  of 
living,  just  where  he  stood.  He  has  grown  up  in  an  environment  where 
words  were  used  to  mask  or  even  deny  reality. 

Mothers  and  Mental  Health:  A  Revised  View 

In  general,  Dr.  Lidz  points  out,  psychoanalytic  theory  has  held  that  if  a 
child  is  to  be  schizophrenic,  something  must  have  gone  wrong  with  the 
mother-child  relationship  at  the  very  beginning.  "We  don't  go  along  with 
that,"  he  says.  "In  these  families,  some  of  the  children  did  not  become 
schizophrenic  even  though  the  early  mother-child  relationship  was  bad. 
Others  became  schizophrenic  even  though  they  did  not  have  particularly 
devastating  experiences  in  early  childhood." 

The  investigators  emphasize  that  the  functions  of  a  mother  extend  over 
many  years  and  that  her  ability  to  carry  them  out  depends  upon  a  variety 
of  emotions,  attitudes,  skills,  and  ways  of  communicating,  which  have 
been  shaped  by  her  relationships  with  her  own  parents  and  brothers  and 
sisters  and  with  her  husband.  The  first  year  or  so  of  a  child's  life  is  only 
one  of  the  critical  periods.  Another  occurs  when  he  starts  to  attend 
school.  His  security  then  depends  upon  having  a  firm  base  at  home  from 
which  he  can  move  outward,  and  this  base  includes  a  mother  who  can 
encourage  him  to  surmount  the  inevitable  difficulties  rather  than  convey 
her  anxieties  or  her  own  distrust  of  the  larger  world. 

Another  critical  period  occurs  at  puberty.  For  a  child  to  develop  into 
an  emotionally  healthy  adult,  Dr.  Lidz  holds  mothers-and  fathers  as  well- 
must  have  the  proper  attitudes  in  regard  to  a  child's  changing  needs  over 
the  first  two  decades  of  life. 

Question:  If  a  mother  has  trouble  getting  along  with  a  child  who  later 
develops  schizophrenia,  may  not  the  original  difficulty  have  been  caused 
by  some  inherent  deficiency  in  the  child?  The  investigators  remark  that 
this  suggestion  is  often  advanced  and  that  it  does  seem  to  hold  true  with 
certain  autistic  children.  But  no  evidence  to  support  it  was  found  in  the 
cases  studied.  Rather,  there  seems  to  have  been  "a  disharmony  in  the 
mother-child  relationship."  The  disharmony  arose  because  of  the  mother's 
own  difficulties,  those  of  her  husband,  and  the  problems  of  the  marriage. 

Psychiatry  has  been  overemphasizing  the  role  of  mothers  in  the 
development  of  schizophrenia,  the  study  suggests,  because  "the  patho- 
logical characteristics  of  some  of  these  mothers  make  a  lasting  impression 
upon  the  psychiatrists  whom  they  harass,"  with  the  result  that  the 
psychiatrists  tend  to  generalize  from  these  mothers  to  all  mothers  of 
schizophrenic  patients. 

Everything  considered,  women  probably  do  have  a  more  important  role 
than  men  in  the  production  of  schizophrenic  children,  the  investigators 
suggest.  For  one  thing,  women  have  a  greater  influence  upon  children.  For 
another,  statistics  indicate  that  schizophrenia  develops  somewhat  later  in 
women  than  in  men.  Since  women  can  remain  sheltered  longer,  this  may 

277 


mean  that  more  mentally  disorganized  women  will  marry.  But  in  general 
the  investigators  believe  that  it  takes  two  persons  to  produce  a  child  and 
two  to  produce  the  distortions  in  family  structure  and  communications 
that  can  make  a  child  schizophrenic. 


The  Parents'  Backgrounds 

The  clinical  workers  in  this  study  had  an  unusual  expeiience:  they  got 
to  know  the  parents  of  schizophrenics  and  in  many  cases  to  help  them, 
and  they  wound  up  feeling  as  much  sympathy  for  them  as  for  the 
patients. 

Among  the  parents  were  a  number  of  individuals  who  weie  pursuing 
successful  careers  in  the  fields  of  business,  industry,  education,  and  the 
arts.  Fifteen  of  the  seventeen  families  came  from  the  upper  socioeconomic 
classes.  As  long  as  relationships  with  other  people  were  fairly  formal,  Dr. 
Lidz  explains,  even  the  most  peculiar  and  disturbed  of  these  individuals 
managed  to  get  along.  It  was  in  close  relationships,  notably  those  within 
the  family,  that  the  peculiarities  became  strikingly  evident. 

These  parents  had  not  tried  to  hurt  their  children,  the  research  team 
reports;  they  had  done  everything  they  could  to  help-everything  within 
their  abilities  and  the  limits  placed  by  their  own  emotional  difficulties. 
"Too  often,"  the  team  observes,  "the  psychiatrist  forgets  his  psychiatric 
understanding  when  dealing  with  parents  and  expects  them  to  have  been 
able  to  be  different  from  what  they  were,  or  to  change  through  reading  a 
book  or  just  because  he  tells  them  to  behave  differently.  They,  too,  areas 
much  bound  to  their  unconscious  conflicts  as  the  patients  and  could  not 
have  been  other  than  what  they  were."  4 

The  problems  of  the  parents  had  been  exacerbated  but  not  caused  by 
the  marriage.  Mrs.  Newcomb  had  grown  up  feeling  unwanted  because  she 
was  a  girl.  Her  father  preached  the  value  of  education  but  refused  to  let 
his  daughter  go  to  college.  He  also  refused  to  let  her  boy  friends  come  into 
the  house.  Mrs.  Newcomb's  mother  had  made  clear  her  feeling  that  her 
own  beauty  and  money  had  been  wasted  on  an  unsatisfactory  marriage. 

The  mother  of  another  boy  who  developed  schizophrenia  was  schizo- 
phrenic herself.  She  was  the  daughter  of  an  anxious,  overly  protective,  and 
confused  woman.  And  this  woman,  the  patient's  grandmother,  had  been 
permitted  to  grow  up  believing  herself  to  be  her  mother's  younger  sister. 
Here  was  evidence,  the  researchers  point  out,  of  serious  pathology  across 
four  generations. 

In  the  case  of  Mrs.  Forel,  who  had  been  dominated  first  by  her  mother 
and  then  by  two  sisters  and  who,  in  turn,  dominated  her  husband,  the 
investigators  found  a  pattern  of  female  domination  going  back  to  her 
great-grandmother. 


'  ^  v"'  *•!""  R"  Fleckl  S"  and  Terry>  Dorothy-  The 
of  the  schizophrenic  patient;  I.  The  Father.  Psychiatry,  1957,  20,  4, 


278 


Another  mother  recalled  that  her  fathei  had  been  a  slave  to  routine. 
Every  night  he  would  read  to  her  from  7  to  7:45,  precisely.  He  committed 
suicide  when  she  was  6. 

The  husbands,  too,  aie  reported  to  have  had  difficult  backgrounds.  One 
man's  father  had  been  an  alcoholic.  Another  husband  was  closely  tied, 
even  after  years  of  marriage,  to  an  almost  psychotic  mothei .  The  father  of 
Mr.  Grau,  the  man  who  became  irrational  on  the  subject  of  Catholicism, 
was  described  as  having  been  stubborn,  dominating,  and  perhaps  paranoid. 
Mr.  Newcomb,  grateful  for  any  crumb  of  affection  his  wife  could  offer, 
was  an  orphan. 

"We  almost  feel,"  says  Dr.  Lidz,  "that  schizophrenia  is  the  end  result  of 
cultural  deviation  increasing  gradually  over  the  generations.  Then  you  get 
two  paients  who  are  unable  to  straighten  out  each  other's  distortions— and 
you  end  up  with  a  child  so  deviant  that  he  virtually  leaves  society." 

So  what  can  be  done? 

"That  is  essentially  what  the  mental  health  movement  is  about,"  the 
investigator  answers,  ".  .  .  to  stop  this  vicious  ciicle  of  unstable  parents 
having  unstable  children."  Among  the  approaches,  he  lists  premarital 
counseling  "to  try  to  keep  some  of  these  people  from  getting  married "- 
an  effort  that  is  not  usually  successful— and  marital  counseling,  to  help 
husbands  and  wives  better  understand  each  other  and  themselves.  Another 
approach  is  to  educate  paients  about  bringing  up  children.  "The  parents 
will  still  have  their  emotional  problems,"  he  observes,  "but  they  will  do 
better  with  their  children  simply  because  of  knowing  what  to  do."  He 
believes  there  should  be  more  marriage  counseling  centers  and  more 
family  planning  and  well-baby  clinics,  and  wider  use  made  of  them. 


The  Brothers  and  Sisters 

The  investigating  team  had  set  itself  two  major  questions: 

First,  what  factors  in  the  family  environment  are  responsible  for,  or 
contributory  to,  the  development  of  schizophrenia? 

Second,  if  certain  factors  in  the  family  environment  lead  to  schizo- 
phrenia in  a  particular  child,  why  are  the  brothers  and  sisters  not  affected? 

The  answers  put  forward  to  the  first  question  have  been  summarized  in 
preceding  sections.  The  Yale  group's  belief  in  their  validity  is  reinforced 
by  the  findings,  noted  below,  reported  as  answers  to  the  second. 

1.  Most  of  the  brothers  and  sisters  of  the  children  who  became  schizo- 
phrenic developed  serious  problems  themselves. 

The  patients  in  the  study  had  24  brothers  and  sisters.  Three  of  these, 
investigators  found,  were  also  schizophrenic.  Seven  others  were  considered 
to  be  borderline  schizophrenics  and  eight  others  to  be  emotionally 
disturbed.  Only  6  of  the  24  were  judged  to  be  either  adequately  or  well 
adjusted,  and  all  but  1  of  these,  the  investigators  report,  suffered  from 
constricted  personalities,  marked  by  limits  on  their  emotional  maturity, 
their  perceptiveness,  and  the  use  of  their  intellectual  resources. 

279 


2.  The  children  who  became  schizophrenic  had  been  brought  up 
circumstances  that  differed  from  those  affecting  their  brothers  and  sisters 

In  half  the  cases  the  circumstances  were  markedly  different,  Dw 
patient,  for  example,  had  been  conceived  during  a  brief  reconciliation 
between  his  parents  1 1  years  after  his  mother -Mrs.  Forel— had  ended  the 
marriage  relationship.  He  was  mothered  by  his  father  and  an  older  biotliei 
and  sister.  After  Mrs.  Forel  was  injured  in  an  accident,  hci  husband 
became  more  subservient  to  her  than  ever  and  she  became  more  opcnlj 
contemptuous  of  him.  The  older  children  left  home  when  the  boy  was 6, 
and  the  father  died  when  he  was  1 1 .  Mrs.  Forel  then  went  to  live  with  om 
of  her  beloved  sisters,  who  forced  her  to  boaid  the  child  elsewhere.  Tilt 
Forels  offered  the  most  dramatic  example  of  changing  circumstances,  and 
even  children  close  together  in  age  and  of  the  same  sex  were  found  to  haw 
been  subjected  to  different  influences. 

Nancy  Grau  was  caught  in  the  middle  of  the  conflict  between  lur 
parents,  mainly  over  religion.  Ellen,  born  less  than  2  years  later,  escaped 
the  woist  of  the  battle— paitly  because  Mr,  Grau  assumed  that  his  wife 
would  not  dare  let  Ellen  become  a  Catholic  and  partly  because  Hltcji 
skillfully  avoided  situations  that  she  knew,  from  watching  Nnncy,  would 
bring  down  trouble. 

Even  a  pair  of  identical  twins  faced  different  developmental  pressures 
As  the  investigators  explain  it,  the  mother  came  to  prefer  Peter,  who  had 
been  born  first,  and  to  identify  him  with  her  envied  twin  sister,  who  also 
had  been  born  first.  Through  this  boy  the  mother  fancied  she  would  liv 
out  the  dominant,  aggressive  role  to  which  she  had  always  aspired,  Ih? 
other  twin,  Philip,  came  to  represent  the  passive  and  feminine  aspects  sli? 
despised  in  herself. 

Peter  grew  up  to  be  grandiose,  antisocial,  in  constant  need  of  admit* 
tion,  and  unconsciously  terrified  of  all  women  because  he  viewed  Hicnih 
overpowering  and  engulfing,  like  his  mother.  He  struggled  wilhudikinini 
common  in  adolescents  who  become  schizophrenic,  the  researches 
In  that  he  was  supposed  to  be  a  part  of  his  mother-to  achieve 
order  to  complete  her  life— and  at  the  same  time  was  supposed  lolwi 
man.  He  chose  to  go  to  a  distant  college  in  order  to  gel  away  from  h 
mother's  seductive  behavior*  When  his  mother  then  turned  lowiinl  Ph-i 
however,  Peter  felt  betrayed,  "and  murderous  impulses  mingled  with  if: 
incestuous,  creating  panic.'1  He  tried  to  fill  his  need  lor  love  in  ;i  hom> 
sexual  relationship  but  lost  his  partner  to  another  boy. 

Philip,  on  the  other  hand,  early  relinquished  his  mother  to  Peter,  Ihi 
investigators  explain,  and  thus  protected  himself  from  her 
Peter's  hostility.  This  twin,  too,  tried  homosexuality,  but  in  Im  ft 
he  was  a  girl— and  therefore  not  in  competition  with  his  brother  and  hi 
wandered  about  the  town  in  girls'  clothing. 

Peter  became  psychotic  at  college  and  had  to  be  hospHaltocd.  Phil; 
broke  down,  too~in  part,  the  investigators  explain,  because  ho  nceJd 
Peter  to  bear  the  brunt  of  the  mother's  attcntion-but  was 
treated  while  living  at  home* 

280 


3.  As  a  group,  children  of  the  same  sex  as  the  child  who  became 
schizophrenic  were  clearly  more  disturbed  than  children  of  the  opposite 
sex, 

The  nine  male  patients  in  the  study  had  eight  biothers  and  six  sisters. 
The  only  healthy  sibling  was  a  sister.  The  16  female  patients  had  7  sisters 
and  3  brotheis.  Two  of  the  brotheis  but  only  one  of  the  sisters  weie 
emotionally  stable. 

These  findings  are  explained,  the  investigators  believe,  by  the  findings 
reported  earlier -the  failure  of  the  parents  to  maintain  their  appropriate 
sex-linked  roles.  The  fathers  of  the  boys  formed  poor  models  for  their 
sons;  the  mothers  of  the  girls,  poor  models  for  the  daughters.  One  or 
more,  and  commonly  all  three,  of  the  following  reasons  were  involved: 
The  parent's  own  serious  psychopathology;  the  parent's  unnatural 
attitude—intrusive,  rivalrous,  or  aloof —toward  the  child ;  a  parent's  loss  of 
worth  resulting  from  the  other  parent's  depreciating  attitudes  and 
behavior. 

Under  the  circumstances,  the  Yale  group  explains,  a  child  who  wanted 
the  approval  and  affection  of  the  parent  of  the  opposite  sex,  tried  to 
differentiate  himself  from,  rather  than  identify  himself  with,  the  parent  of 
the  same  sex.  Thus  the  child  lacked  a  model  to  follow  in  order  to  gain 
maturity  as  a  man  or  a  woman. 

At  the  same  time,  the  parent  of  the  opposite  sex,  to  whom  the  child 
was  naturally  diawn,  produced  further  confusion  by  conveying  a  distorted 
picture  of  that  sex.  Mothers  piesented  sons  with  a  model  of  women  as 
persons  clangeious  to  males;  fatheis  presented  daughters  with  a  model  of 
men  as  unworthy  of  or  even  dangerous  to  females. 

It  follows  that  ccitain  types  of  family  structure  and  interaction  will  be 
more  dangerous  to  a  child  of  one  sex  than  to  a  child  of  the  other.  A 
mother  who  cannot  establish  clear  boundaries  between  herself  and  the 
child  will  interfere  more  seriously  with  a  boy's  development  than  a  girl's. 
This  is  because  a  boy,  to  achieve  a  firm  masculine  identity,  must  break 
away  more  completely  than  a  girl  from  the  initial  mother-child  symbiosis, 
Likewise  a  weak  father  is  worse  for  a  boy  than  a  girl  because  a  boy  needs 
to  identify  himself  with  a  masculine  figure. 

On  the  other  hand,  a  cold  and  aloof  mother  does  more  harm  to  a  girl 
than  to  a  boy  because  a  girl  needs  to  absorb  maternal  feelings  in  order  to 
develop  maternal  characteristics  herself.  Likewise  a  father  who  dominates 
and  belittles  the  mother  interferes  more  seriously  with  a  girl's  develop- 
ment than  a  boy's.  In  this  case  the  girl,  seeking  her  father's  affection,  must 
differentiate  herself  from  the  woman  he  finds  unsatisfactory  instead  of 
pursuing  the  normal  course  and  endeavoring  to  emulate  the  woman  he 
loves. 

Published  descriptions  of  the  parents  of  schizophrenic  children  often 
seem  contradictory,  the  investigators  report,  but  the  contradictions  may 
be  reconcilable  if  the  patient's  sex  is  taken  into  account.  "Firm  sexual 
identity— that  is,  to  behave  as  it  is  appropriate  for  a  member  of  one's  own 
sex  to  behave-is  one  of  the  strongest  foundations  of  a  stable  personality," 

281 


the  study  says.  "A  child  who  grows  up  insecure  in  his  essential  identity  is 
likely  to  be  in  tiouble." 

In  sum,  if  family  conditions  are  such  that  one  child  becomes  schizo- 
phrenic, this  study  finds,  other  children  are  more  likely  to  be  affected 
deleteriously  if  they  are  of  the  same  sex  as  the  patient.  Not  all  Hie 
affected  children  become1  schizophrenic,  because  family  pressures  differ 
from  child  to  child,  and  also  because  the  child  who  becomes  schizophrenic 
often  lessens  the  impact  of  the  parental  disturbance  upon  the  other 
children-by  seiving  as  a  target,  for  instance,  or  as  an  example  of  how  not 
to  get  along. 


The  Patients  Improve 

Of  the  nine  male  patients  in  the  study,  only  one  is  still  hospitali/ed, 
elsewhere.  The  others  are  reported  to  be  fending  for  themselves,  most  of 
them  quite  well.  However,  one  young  man  who  went  back  to  college  after 
several  years  of  hospitaliz-ation  and  another  who  is  working  are  noi 
considered  very  successful  patients. 

The  young  women  haven't  done  so  well.  The  investigators  don't  know 
why  but  call  attention  to  a  finding  by  Dr.  David  Rosenthal  of  NIMH,  thai 
schizophrenia  tends  to  become  more  chronic  in  women  than  men.  The 
research  group  notes  also  that  five  of  the  eight  female  patients  in  the 
study  were  rather  chionic  cases  when  fast  hospitalized-' 'the  parents 
being  less  alert  to  their  abnormalities  or  less  apt  to  secure  optimal  euro." 
Two  or  three  of  the  young  women  still  have  to  be  hospitalized  from  thno 
to  time. 

In  the  group's  judgment,  only  a  few  of  the  patients  would  be  good  fouls 
as  parents  themselves;  it  is  hoped  that  most  of  them  won't  have  children. 

Implications  for  Therapy 

The  investigators  believe  that  they  themselves  and  the  doctors  as- 
sociated with  them  are  now  better  able  to  treat  schizophrenia  because  Ik 
study  has  made  them  more  aware  of  the  results  of  family  interrelations. 
The  patients'  problems  and  communications  have  become  much  mow 
comprehensible.  As  Dr.  Flecks  puts  it:  "We  now  understand  quickly 
things  that  before  this  study  might  have  taken  us  months  to  figure  oul." 

As  the  result  of  this  work  and  of  research  elsewhere  reaching  the  sanw 
general  conclusion  that  schizophrenics  have  abnormal  family  backgrounds, 
the  research  team  expects  to  see  an  increasing  emphasis  on  efforts  lo 
change  a  patient's  environment. 

The  tendency  at  the  Yale  Psychiatric  Institute  has  been  to  keep  schizo- 
phrenics long  enough  so  that  when  they  leave  the  hospital  they  can  work 
and  live  on  their  own,  away  from  the  families  in  which  they  were  brought 
up.  But  the  Institute  also  tries  to  change  the  environment  by  changing  Ifw 
parents  through  psychotherapy-individual,  family,  parents  alone,  and 
group.  Dr.  Lidz  believes  group  therapy,  in  which  parents  of  schizophrenic 

282 


children  meet  together,  to  be  particularly  effective.  It  lessens  the  parents' 
feeling  of  guilt,  gets  them  over  the  difficult  period  of  adjusting  to  their 
child's  hospitalization,  enables  them  to  see  what  has  gone  wrong  in  other 
families  and  therefore  what  may  have  gone  wrong  in  their  own  and  what 
they  can  try  to  do  about  it.  Where  patients  can  be  hospitalized  only  a 
month  or  so,  he  points  out,  group  therapy  offers  virtually  the  only  hope 
of  effecting  an  environmental  change. 

On  the  basis  of  his  experience  during  the  study,  Dr.  Lidz  doubts  that 
any  great  change  can  be  worked  in  the  things  basically  wrong  with  the 
family.  The  main  hope  lies  in  changing  attitudes— those  of  the  parents 
toward  the  patient  and  those  of  the  patient  toward  the  parents.  "In  the 
case  of  the  patient,"  he  says,  "our  main  effort  is  to  free  him  of  the 
obligation  to  feel  and  think  as  he  did  in  the  past,  in  order  to  satisfy  the 
needs  of  a  disturbed  parent  or  to  model  himself  on  that  parent."  If  the 
patient  had  been  able  to  see  his  family  truly  and  to  accept  its  peculiarities 
without  becoming  enmeshed  in  them,  Dr.  Lidz  believes,  he  would  not 
have  become  sick. 

The  psychiatrists  who  conducted  the  study  say  they  would  now  prefer 
to  keep  young  patients  hospitalized  a  longer  rather  than  a  shorter  time— 
"because  we  have  come  to  realize  that  the  young  schizophrenic  has  a  great 
deal  to  learn,  mainly  about  how  to  get  along  with  people.  In  a  way,  he 
now  has  to  go  to  school— a  school  for  resocialization— and  he  cannot  finish 
in  a  couple  of  months." 

In  numerous  other  mental  institutions  the  effort  is  to  get  the  patient 
out  of  the  hospital  as  soon  as  possible.  "You  hear  it  said  again  and  again 
that  if  we  use  drugs,  we  can  get  the  patients  back  in  the  community  in  a 
short  time  and  they  can  get  along,"  the  investigators  note.  "That's  true,  to 
an  extent.  At  our  general  hospital,  Yale-New  Haven,  we  are  interested  in 
that  approach.  We  feel  some  things  can  be  modified— perhaps  the  parents* 
attitude  toward  the  child  and  toward  each  other -so  that  the  patient  can 
go  home  and  get  along  with  outpatient  care." 

"But  here  at  the  Institute  we  aren't  very  excited  about  doing  that. 
We're  not  interested  in  keeping  patients  out  of  the  hospital,  nor  in  sending 
them  back  home  fast,  into  what  we  feel  is  a  pathogenic  environment. 
We're  interested  in  finding  out  how  we  can  get  them  to  lead  reasonably 
satisfactory  lives.  And  that's  a  very  different  goal." 

"You  see,*'  Dr.  Lidz  adds,  "there  are  different  ways  of  treating 
different  patients,  according  to  the  conditions  surrounding  them  and  ac- 
cording to  what  can  be  provided  for  them." 

The  research  group  sees  another  implication  in  its  findings:  that  psy- 
chiatrists should  take  a  different  attitude  toward  the  parents  and  not 
consider  them  either  malicious  or  downright  rejecting. 


Birth  and  Progress  of  a  Research  Project 

As  a  medical  student  (College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons,  Columbia 
University)  and  an  intern  (New  Haven  Hospital  and  National  Hospital, 

283 


London),  Theodore  Lidz  recalls  having  been  particularly  interested  in 
birth  disoiders  affecting  the  central  nervous  system.  These  were  organic 
disorders;  damage  or  rnaldevelopment  could  be  demonstrated  01  surmised. 
He  became  impressed  with  the  idea  that  schizophrenia  was  "a  different 
kind  of  biith  disorder." 

The  idea  was  strengthened  during  his  psychiatry  residency  at  the  Johns 
Hopkins  Hospital,  1938-41,  under  the  late  Adolph  Meyer,  one  of  America's 
great  psychiatrists.  Dr.  Lidz  helped  care  for  Dr.  Meyer's  private  patients, 
"I  got  to  know  the  relatives  of  the  patients  quite  well,"  he  recalls,  "and 
became  impressed  how  really  disturbed  many  of  them  were.  The  more  1 
listened  to  them,  the  more  certain  it  appeared  that  an  individual  in  those 
families  couldn't  possibly  have  grown  up  to  be  a  stable  person.  Some  of 
the  siblings  felt  the  same  way  and  told  me  why  they  thought  they  were 
not  schizophrenic  but  the  patient  was." 

Dr.  Lidz  wrote  a  paper  on  five  of  the  patients  and  their  families  and  was 
thinking  of  sending  it  off  for  publication  when  he  noticed  that  the  initials 
of  their  last  names  went  L,  M,  N,  0,  and  P.  "I  thought  that  if  the  initials 
could  occur  this  way  by  coincidence,"  he  relates,  "it  could  also  be  coin- 
cidence that  the  families  of  my  schizophrenic  patients  were  disturbed." 

So  he  and  his  wife-~Dr.  Ruth  Lidz,  whom  he  describes  as  a  congenital 
psychiatrist,  inasmuch  as  her  father  was  piofessor  of  psychiatry  at  Heidel- 
berg—set out  to  learn  whether  or  not  family  environments  were  worth 
investigating  for  their  role  in  the  development  of  schizophrenia.  They 
compared  the  histories  of  the  latest  50  schizophrenic  patients  admitted  to 
the  Phipps  Clinic  at  the  Johns  Hopkins  with  those  of  the  latest  50  manic- 
depressive  patients.  Even  though  the  histories  were  far  from  complete,  the 
husband-and-wife  team  found  marked  differences  in  the  family  back- 
grounds of  the  two  types  of  patients. 

Publication  of  the  findings  was  delayed  8  years.  The  first  interruption 
was  the  war,  during  which  Lidz  served  as  an  Army  psychiatrist,  rising  to 
be  a  lieutenant  colonel  and  chief  of  the  neuropsychiatric  service  of  the 
18th  General  Hospital.  Later,  as  the  Lidzcs  compared  notes  with  psy- 
chiatrist friends  who,  like  themselves,  were  treating  schizophrenia,  the 
observation  that  schizophrenics  come  from  disturbed  families  "seemed  so 
obvious  that  we  saw  no  reason  for  reporting  it."  Then  they  realized  that 
while  the  finding  might  be  obvious,  nothing  like  it  had  been  published.  So 
in  1949  out  came  the  results  of  the  study  the  Liclzes  had  done  in  1941.5 

Dr.  Lidz  went  to  New  Haven  in  1951,  determined  to  undertake  the 
present  study  if  he  could  find  financial  support,  which  he  did  2  years  later 
from  NIMH.  (The  other  Dr.  Lidz  went  to  New  Haven,  too,  mainly  as  wife 
and  mother  and  assisting  in  the  study  only  part  time,  also  maintaining  an 
office  at  home  as  a  practicing  psychiatrist.) 

Dr.  Fleck,  who  had  worked  with  Dr.  Lidz  at  the  Johns  Hopkins  Hos- 
pital, left  his  teaching  position  and  part-time  practice  in  Seattle  and  joined 
Dr.  Lidz  at  Yale  to  pursue  this  study  and  carry  out  their  ideas  on  how  a 


5Lidx,  Ruth  W.,  and  Lidz,  T.  The  family  environment  of  schizophrenic  patients. 
Amer.  J,  Psychiat.,  1949,  332-345. 

284 


mental  hospital  should  be  conducted.  Dr.  Fleck  assumed  responsibility  for 
the  hospital.  Miss  Cornelison,  a  social  worker,  took  the  major  respon- 
sibility for  assisting  the  parents  and  gaining  information  about  the 
families.  Typically,  a  research  project  begins  with  the  formulation  of  a 
specific  hypothesis  and  continues  with  attempts  to  demonstrate  its 
validity —thro ugh  experimentation,  observation,  and  other  types  of  study. 
But  Dr.  Lidz  explains  that  in  this  case  it  seemed  wise  to  start  with  only  a 
very  general  hypothesis  and  to  explore  the  field.  "We  might  have  set  up 
the  hypothesis  that  schizophrenia  is  caused  by  a  certain  type  of  mother, 
for  a  number  of  observations  seemed  to  point  in  that  direction,"  he  says. 
"But  then  we  should  have  been  studying  mothers,  and  we  had  reason  to 
suspect  that  fathers  also  were  important.  In  the  beginning  we  just  had  the 
idea— look,  children  grow  up  in  families;  then  thinking  has  to  be  deter- 
mined to  a  certain  extent  by  what  goes  on  in  the  family;  they  need  certain 
kinds  of  care  in  the  family.  We  also  knew  that  most,  if  not  all,  schizo- 
phrenics grow  up  in  disturbed  homes.  We  sought  to  learn  if  something  in 
the  radius  of  the  family  might  be  responsible." 

The  investigators  had  given  themselves  5  or  6  years.  (As  in  most  such 
cases,  the  research  job  for  the  senioi  membeis  of  the  team  was  only  part 
time.  The  first  obligation  was  to  run  the  44-bed  psychiatric  hospital.  They 
also  taught.  The  proportion  of  their  time  they  could  give  to  research  was 
never  as  much  as  50  percent  and  frequently  was  much  lower.)  But  the 
exploration  pointed  to  so  many  potential  sources  of  trouble  that  Drs.  Lidz 
and  Fleck  wondered  if  they  would  ever  identify  the  essential  ones. 

Everywhere  they  looked  they  discovered  something  amiss.  Usually  both 
parents  were  disturbed,  but  generally  one  of  them  more  than  the  other. 
Some  of  the  patients  had  lacked  proper  nurturance  during  infancy  but  all 
of  them  had  lacked  it  later  on.  Some  of  the  families  had  been  in  an  almost 
constant  tempestuous  state;  others  had  been  relatively  though  only  super- 
ficially calm.  Communications  within  families  were  distorted,  though  in 
different  ways. 

Another  problem,  in  addition  to  the  confusing  wealth  of  material, 
popped  up.  As  patients  selected  for  the  study  began  improving,  they 
began  showing  hostility  to  their  parents,  and  the  paicnts  reacted  by  trying 
to  take  them  out  of  the  hospital.  The  team  lost  several  subjects  just  as  it 
felt  it  was  beginning  to  know  the  family.  It  solved  the  problem  by  telling 
the  parents  ahead  of  time  what  to  expect  and  by  providing  them  with  the 
emotional  support  they  needed. 

The  investigators  had  originally  dared  hope  they  would  find  a  specific 
cause  of  schizophrenia.  Instead,  to  fit  their  obseivations,  they  found  it 
necessary  to  develop  ideas  about  the  structure  and  functions  of  the 
family.  "It  didn't  take  us  12  years  to  collect  our  material,"  Dr.  Lidz 
remarks.  "Much  of  the  time  went  in  trying  to  find  a  way  of  thinking 
about  it— in  trying  to  develop  concepts.  We  also  had  to  learn  a  great  deal 
about  how  families  function  and  the  functions  of  families." 

When  the  parents  fail  to  establish  the  essential  structure  and  fail  to 
carry  out  the  essential  functions,  the  investigators  concluded,  schizo- 
phrenia can  develop  in  the  offspring  most  affected  by  the  failures.  The 

285 


chcumstances  leading  to  these  failures,  but  not  the  general  nature  of  the 
failmes  themselves,  differ  fiom  family  to  family. 

The  exploiation,  then,  has  led  to  the  hypothesis  that  schizophrenia 
develops  as  the  result  of  "deficiencies  in  family  organization  and  function- 
ing as  noted  in  failuies  of  the  family's  nurturant  capacities,  structure,  and 
capacities  to  transmit  the  essential  adaptive  techniques." 

"The  patients  in  the  study  had  not  gained  the  essentials  for  existence  as 
independent  adults,"  the  investigators  sum  up.  "They  had  not  gained 
adequate  trust  in  otheis,  confidence  in  themselves,  a  stable  personality 
organization  and  structure,  or  the  linguistic  tools  they  needed  in  order  to 
think  clearly  for  self-guidance,  to  understand  others,  and  to  relate  to 
others  in  cooperative  endeavors.  With  such  deficits,  their  integration  and 
adaptation  were  bound  to  be  highly  tenuous." 

The  research  group  believes  that  the  case  histories  of  the  families 
studied-and  also  the  histories  of  other  schizophrenics  treated  by  the  same 
investigators-support  the  hypothesis.  But  the  group  recognizes  that 
important  questions  remain  to  be  answered. 


A  Study  of  Sociopaths 

If  the  hypothesis  is  correct,  the  group  observes,  abnormalities  in  the 
families  of  children  who  are  emotionally  disturbed  but  not  schizophrenic 
should  differ  from  those  of  the  families  that  have  been  discussed.  As  a 
partial  check  of  its  findings,  therefore,  the  team  instituted  a  study  of 
individuals  described  by  some  psychiatrists  as  sociopaths  and  by  others  as 
persons  with  a  character  disorder.  These  persons  have  calloused  con- 
sciences or,  as  Dr.  Lidz  prefers  to  put  it,  consciences  that  operate  ineffec- 
tually. The  behavior  of  such  persons  bothers  society  but  not,  apparently, 
themselves. 

The  investigators  looked  for  sociopaths  from  families  at  the  upper 
socio-economic  levels— as  were  most  of  the  families  in  the  schizophrenic 
study— and  found  them.  By  and  large  the  boys  had  come  to  the  attention 
of  some  public  agency  for  auto  theft,  stealing  from  stores,  or  assault;  tlie 
ghls,  for  sexual  delinquency. 

The  big  problem  in  this  phase  of  the  study  has  been  the  dropout  rate, 
The  parents  have  cooperated  so  begrudgingly,  in  general,  that  the  re- 
searchers have  not  gotten  to  know  that  families  with  anything  like  the 
thoroughness  desired.  However,  from  a  preliminary  study  of  the  material 
now  available,  the  psychiatrists  report  themselves  reasonably  sure  that  the 
two  sets  of  families  do  indeed  differ,  Superficially,  at  least,  the  parents  of 
a  soclopathic  child  seem  to  be  marked  by  these  characteristics; 

1.  An  unawareness  of  what  the  child  has  been  up  to.  As  an  extreme 
example,  one  boy  sneaked  a  girl  into  the  house  and  kept  her  in  his  room  a 
week,  with  his  parents  completely  unaware, 

2,  A  willingness  to  accept  sociopathic  behavior  until  the  situation 
reaches  the  point  where  the  police,  or  some  other  agent  of  society,  steps 

286 


in.  For  example,  one  mother  knew  that  her  boy  kept  stolen  money  in  the 
house,  but  she  did  nothing  because,  as  she  explained,  she  was  afraid  of 
getting  him  into  trouble. 

3.  A  concern  with  appearances  rather  than  more  basic  values. 

4.  Often  a  disregard  of  social  and  ethical  values. 

One  underlying  fault  in  the  families  of  sociopaths,  the  investigators 
suspect,  may  turn  out  to  be  that  the  parents  have  abdicated  authoiity  to 
the  children.  This  too,  would  be  a  breaching  of  the  boundaries  between 
generations,  but  quite  different  from  the  one  found  to  occur  in  the 
families  of  schizophrenics. 


A  Research  Problem:  Selecting  Families  for  a  Long-Term  Study 

Dis  Lidz  and  Fleck  would  like  to  test  and  elaborate  their  hypothesis 
about  the  causes  of  schizophrenia  by  closely  observing  families  in  which 
schizophrenia  is  considered  likely  to  develop.  Such  a  predictive  or 
longitudinal  study,  they  point  out,  would  turn  up  more  nearly  accurate 
information  about  family  interactions  during  the  early  lives  of  the 
children  than  even  the  most  painstaking  restrospective  study,  such  as  the 
one  just  ending. 

But  how  do  you  select  the  families?  If  you  make  a  blind  start,  observing 
any  new  families  that  agree  to  cooperate,  Dr.  Lidz  points  out,  you  may 
not  get  the  job  done  in  a  lifetime.  Figuring  the  incidence  of  schizopmenia 
at  something  more  than  1  percent  and  figuring  3  children  to  a  family,  he 
notes,  you  would  have  to  take  in  about  30  families  in  order  to  get  1  likely 
to  have  a  schizophrenic  offspring.  "Maybe  the  moie  disturbed  families 
would  avoid  coming  into  the  project,"  the  investigator  goes  on,  "so  you 
could  very  well  study  50  families  and  not  come  up  with  a  schizophrenic. 
If  I'm  to  study  families  for  15  01  20  years,  I  need  greater  assurance  that 
Pm  dealing  with  a  high-risk  population." 

Hcie  Dr.  Lidz  puts  his  finger  on  one  of  the  major  problems  of  clinical 
research:  the  comparative  slowness  with  which  human  beings  develop  and 
therefore  the  length  of  time  that  must  be  given  to  such  studies  as  the 
relation  between  aberrant  developmental  factors  and  schizophrenia,  or 
other  mental  disorders. 

Like  other  investigators  facing  this  problem,  Dr.  Lidz  is  looking  for  a 
shortcut-in  his  case,  some  screening  procedure  for  selecting  high-risk 
families  so  that  the  ratio  of  families  studied  to  families  in  which  schizo- 
phrenia develops  is  not  greater  than  that  about  5  to  1 .  He  is  hopeful  that 
current  work  with  a  test  of  thought  disorders  in  families  that  have 
produced  a  schizophrenic  child  will  provide  the  answer. 


A  Possible  Solution:  Testing  for  Disordered  Styles  of  Thinking 

The  finding  that  schizophrenic  patients  had  been  exposed  all  their  lives 
to  disordered  and  irrational  modes  of  thinking  and  communicating  within 

287 


the  family  gave  the  research  group  a  number  of  questions;  (1)  Is  dis- 
ordered thinking  indeed  a  characteristic  of  all  families  producing  a  schizo- 
phrenic offspring?  (2)  Will  a  simple  test  for  disordered  thinking  distinguish 
these  families  from  normal  families?  (3)  Will  it  distinguish  them  from 
families  in  which  a  child  develops  a  psychiatric  problem  other  than 
schizophrenia?  (4)  Can  families  In  which  schizophienia  is  likely  to  develop 
be  spotted  beforehand  through  such  a  test7 

Recent  woik  by  Dr.  Margaret  Singer,  the  psychologist  who  was 
mentioned  earlier,  and  Dr.  Lyinan  Wynne  at  the  National  Institute  of 
Mental  Health  provides  evidence,  in  addition  to  that  of  the  Yale  group, 
that  the  answer  to  the  first  question  is  yes.  Using  projective  test  material 
gathered  at  NIMH  and  at  Yale,  those  investigators  found  that  disordered 
thinking  in  schizophrenic  offspring  was  related  to  peculiarities  in  the 
styles  of  thinking  and  communicating  of  the  parents.  It  proved  possible  on 
the  basis  of  the  projective  tests,  all  by  themselves,  to  differentiate  parents 
of  patients  from  parents  of  normal  persons.  Because  the  type  of  peculiarity 
differed  from  family  to  family,  it  was  even  possible  to  say  which  patient 
was  the  offspirng  of  which  parents. 

The  Yale  group  meanwhile  has  been  working  with  a  simpler  instrument 
known  as  the  Object  Sorting  Test.  In  the  first  part  of  this  test  a  person  is 
given  something-such  as  a  toy,  a  dish,  a  pack  of  matches-and  directed  to 
place  it  on  a  table  along  with  whatever  other  objects  on  the  table  belong 
with  it.  Then  he  is  asked  why  all  the  objects  in  a  group  belong  together. 
This  process  is  repeated  a  number  of  times.  In  the  second  part  of  the  test 
the  person  is  shown  in  succession  a  number  of  different  groups,  all  tlie 
objects  in  a  given  group  having  something  in  common— they  are  all  red,  for 
example,  or  all  toys,  or  ail  smoking  materials.  Each  time  the  person  is 
asked  why  the  objects  belong  together.  Everything  he  says  during  the  test 
is  recorded. 

This  version  of  the  test  is  the  one  developed  a  dozen  years  ago  by  an 
Australian  psychologist,  S.H.  Lovibond,  and  used  with  schizophrenics, 
The  scoring  system  was  based  on  the  idea  that  in  schizophrenic  thinking  a 
person  is  unable  to  suppress,  out  of  all  the  material  that  comes  to  mind, 
the  unessential  irrelevant,  and  illogical.  In  the  Object  Sorting  Test,  such  a 
person  might  group  a  toy  cigar  and  a  ball  together  because  both  were 
made  of  rubber,  but  he  might  then  add  the  packet  of  matches  to  the 
group  because  matches  can  be  used  to  light  cigars.  The  highest  scoring 
patients-that  is,  those  who  most  often  made  inappropriate  groupings- 
were  found  to  be  those  whose  schizophrenia  had  been  rated  most  severe. 
Later,  another  Australian  investigator  used  the  test  with  parents  of 
schizophrenic  patients.  He  reported  that  60  percent  of  these  parents, 
including  at  least  one  parent  of  each  patient,  scored  high  in  contrast  to 
only  9  percent  of  a  group  of  parents  who  did  not  have  a  schizophrenic 
child. 

This  was  exciting.  When  Dr.  Lidz  and  his  associates  repeated  the 
experiment,  however,  they  got  much  less  striking  results.  Where  was  the 
trouble?  "We  thought  we  could  run  it  down  in  4  months/'  Dr.  Lidz 
recalls,  "but  it  took  4  years.  In  the  beginning  we  couldn't  say  that  the 

288 


people  m  Australia  were  wrong  because,  as  we  went  over  our  techniques 
and  results,  we  found  where  we  ourselves  might  have  been  wrong.  For 
example,  there  was  a  weakness  in  the  contiol  groiip  that  might  possibly 
have  accounted  for  the  difference.  So  we  kept  working,  and  writing  back 
and  forth,  and  eventually  concluded  that  the  investigator  down  there 
hadn't  run  a  proper  control  group  and  hadn't  had  anybody  check  the 
reliability  of  his  scoring. 

"Nevertheless,  there  was  something  in  it— not  as  good  as  he  had  put  it 
but  good  enough  to  be  highly  interesting  to  us.  When  we  finally  finished, 
we  found  that  the  mothers  of  schizophrenic  patients  scored  significantly 
differently  on  this  test  from  the  mothers  of  controls,  but  that  there  was 
no  significant  difference  between  the  two  groups  of  fathers. 

"Our  psychologist  kept  saying,  however,  that  there  really  was  some- 
thing wrong  with  the  way  the  fathers  went  at  this  blamed  thing  and  that  it 
wasn't  being  picked  up  by  this  particular  scoiing  method.  So  then  we 
started  working  up  different  scoring  methods." 

When  Dr.  Singer  demonstrated  that  the  parents  of  schizophrenics  could 
be  identified  on  the  basis  of  projective  tests,  Dr.  Lidz  sent  her  the  records 
of  the  Object  Sorting  Test  as  administered  by  the  Yale  group.  She  was  in 
California.  The  tests  had  been  administered  in  New  Haven  by  other 
psychologists,  and  Dr.  Singer  was  aware  only  of  the  husband-wife  pairings. 
Nevertheless  she  identified  80  percent  of  the  couples  correctly.  On  the 
Object  Sorting  Test  as  on  projective  tests,  she  reported,  the  responses  of 
the  parents  of  schizophrenics  were  marked  by  the  fragmentation  and 
blurring  of  attention  and  meaning. 

Using  Dr.  Singer's  comments,  Dr.  Cynthia  Wild  at  Yale  has  developed  a 
new  means  of  scoring  the  Object  Sorting  Test  for  use  by  the  Yale  group 
and  other  researchers  in  studying  schizophrenic  families.  The  responses  are 
scored  in  several  categories,  of  which  the  first  is  An  Inability  to  Maintain  a 
Consistent  Attitude  to  a  Task.  The  types  of  responses  scored  in  this 
category  are  desciibed  as: 

1.  Fragmentation  of  attention.—  This  includes  the  introduction  of  an 
extraneous   topic   (such  as  a  personal  experience)  or  of  behavior  that 
interferes  with  the  testing  (such  as  getting  up  and  walking  around  the 
room).  It  also  includes  "shifts  of  context  of  reference,"  meaning  that  the 
subject  has  a  piecemeal  approach  to  the  objects  within  one  group  and 
seems   to  shift  fluidly   from   one   frame   of  reference  to   another.   For 
instance,  when  shown  a  circle  of  red  paper,  a  red  eraser,  a  red  rubber  ball, 
and  a  red  book  of  matches,  he  says  that  the  eraser  doesn't  belong  except 
that  it  could  be  used  to  eradicate  anything  written  on  the  paper,  that  the 
matches  could  be  used  to  light  the  paper,  and  that  the  ball  doesn't  belong 
either  except  that,  like  the  eraser,  it  is  rubber. 

Meticulousness  to  a  peculiar  or  bizarre  degree  is  also  scored.  As  one 
example,  a  subject  looks  at  a  group  of  objects,  all  of  them  made  of  rubber, 
and  points  that  the  toy  cigar  has  a  paper  band,  the  eraser  contains 
abrasives,  and  the  sink  stopper  has  a  metal  handle. 

2.  Inability  to  maintain  the  role  of  a  subject  being  tested.  —  Here  the 
subject  wants  to  take  over  the  tester's  role.  He  reaches  out  and  adds 

289 


another  object  to  the  group  the  examiner  has  place  in  front  of  him,  or  he 
badgers,  criticizes,  or  lectures  the  examiner  for  not  following  the  subject's 
idea  of  how  the  test  should  be  given. 

3.  Negativism.— "The  subject  does  not  accept  the  basic  assumption  of 
the  test  that  there  is  some  reason  why  certain  of  the  objects  belong 
together.  Asked  why  a  group  of  round  objects  go  together,  for  example, 
he  says,  "Who  says  they  do!" 

The  second  main  category  is  Blurring  of  Meaning.  Here  a  subject  gives 
the  right  answer,  but  adds  several  otheis  and  doesn't  indicate  which  lie 
considers  best.  Or  he  gives  the  right  one  with  an  air  of  extreme  un- 
certainty or  qualification,  He  does  not  let  any  response  stand.  For 
example,  he  puts  the  red  ball  with  the  red  paper  circle  and  says,  "Both 
reddish  and  round,  and  otherwise  I  don't  see  any  leal  connection  with  (lie 
other  things." 

The  final  main  category  is  Peculiars  It  includes:  (\) peculiar  verbaliza* 
(ions,  such  as  stilted  language  and  made-up  words;  and  (2)  imprecise 
referents,  meaning  vague  statements  that  could  be  applied  to  almost  any 
group  of  objects-for  instance,  a  subject  groups  the  pieces  of  silverware 
because  "you  use  them  together." 

After  satisfying  itself  that  the  scoring  system  was  reliable,  meaning  Unit 
clinical  psychologists,  working  independently,  tended  to  arrive  at  much 
the  same  score  in  each  case,  the  Yale  group  began  applying  it  widely,  Qiw 
of  the  fiist  findings  pointed  to  a  relationship  between  scores  on  the  Object 
Sorting  Test  and  both  age  and  education.  There  was  a  tendency  for  older 
parents  to  make  higher,  or  worse,  scores.  There  was  a  considerably  mom 
marked  tendency  for  parents  with  the  least  education  to  make  the  woisl 
scores. 

"In  these  studies  of  thinking,"  Dr.  Lidz  comments,  "people  with  loss 
than  a  high  school  education  lespond  much  more  poorly  than  we 
expected.  We  believe  this  can  be  explained  by  the  ways  of  thinking  they 
learned.  The  poorly  educated  are  accustomed  to  viewing  things  very 
concretely,  they  have  difficulty  thinking  conceptually.'1 

This  relationship  between  test  scores  and  education,  the  investigator 
suspects,  ties  in  with  the  widely  reported  observation  that  schizophrenia  is 
most  prevelant  among  people  at  the  lower  socioeconomic  levels.  These 
people  are  also  the  ones  with  the  least  education  and  therefore,  Dr.  LidK 
believes,  the  ones  most  likely  to  have  trouble  viewing  their  difficulties  in 
an  organized  way  and  teaching  their  children  to  think  clearly.  (Another 
factor  in  the  greater  incidence  of  schizophrenia  at  the  lower  levels,  Dr. 
Lidz  points  out,  probably  is  the  greater  incidence  of  disorganized 
families.) 

The  new  scoring  system  has  now  been  applied  to  the  transcripts  ol 
approximately  200  parents,  including  some  who  had  been  tested  by 
investigators  at  the  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health.  For  a  clear-cul 
comparison  between  parents  of  schizophrenic  children  and  parents  ol 
apparently  normal  children,  however,  the  subjects  were  matched  on 
education  and  age,  fathers  and  mothers  separately,  a  process  that  cut  the 
sample  to  about  100  individuals. 

290 


Major  findings  follow: 

1 .  Of  the  parents  of  patients,  75  percent  had  high  scores  (above  the 
group's  median);  of  the  control  group  of  parents,  31  percent. 

2.  In   58   percent  of  the  couples  with  schizophrenic  offspring,  both 
parents  had  high  scores.  The   corresponding  figure  in   the  case  of  the 
control  couples  was  12  percent. 

3.  The  two  groups  were  discriminated  better  when  compared  on  the 
basis  of  the  total  scores  for  each  couple.  In  the  patient-parent  group,  79 
percent  of  the  couples  had  high  scores  (as  compared  with  75  percent  of 
the  individuals).  In  the  control  group,  82  percent  of  the  couples  had  low 
scores  (as  compared  with  69  percent  of  the  individuals). 

In  view  of  number  3  and  of  related  findings-notably  that  in  control 
couples  where  one  parent  scored  high,  the  other  almost  invariably  scored 
low— the  investigators  suggest  that  a  healthier  parent  may  offset  the  effect 
of  the  sicker  one,  so  that  the  family  style  of  communication  is  not 
seriously  disturbed. 

The  question  now  is:  Will  the  Object  Sorting  Test  differentiate  the 
parents  of  schizophrenic  children  from  the  paients  of  children  having 
other  serious  emotional  disturbances?  Work  on  this  has  been  started.  If 
the  answer  is  affirmative,  mental  health  researchers  will  have  something 
they  have  long  needed— a  relatively  simple  technique  to  provide  a  first  step 
in  screening  out  families  with  a  high  risk  of  having  a  schizophrenic  child. 
Those  families  can  then  be  observed  through  the  years  to  learn  all  the 
circumstances  entering  into  the  development  of  schizophrenia  in  one  of 
the  children. 


A  Basic  Problem  in  Clinical  Research 

Dr.  Lidz  believes  that  too  little  research  is  being  done  on  schizophrenia, 
apart  from  studies  on  biochemical  or  pharmacological  matteis.  As  he 
states  it,  the  problem  is  circular.  Good  clinical  research  in  schizophrenia 
demands  a  good  facility— that  is,  a  hospital  treating  psychotic  patients- 
having  a  stable  staff  of  experienced  clinicians.  Only  under  such  conditions, 
he  believes,  can  enough  patients  be  studied  intensively  and  long  enough  to 
yield  meaningful  results.  But  most  experienced  clinicians  are  not  prepared 
to  give  the  bulk  of  their  time  to  inpatient  service  and  research,  and  most 
inpatient  service  would  not  begin  to  pay  them  adequately.  Further,  not 
enough  people  are  getting  the  kind  of  training~-"thorough  training,  going 
beyond  a  couple  of  years  of  inpatient  experience" -that  will  enable  them 
to  provide  really  adequate  care  to  psychotic  patients. 

"What  I  think  we  need,"  Dr.  Lidz  says,  "aie  more  places  where  people 
can  work  cooperatively  on  a  given  problem,  as  here  at  the  Yale  Psychiatric 
Institute,  In  such  a  place  one  gets  to  know  patient  after  patient  in  many 
different  situations—not  only  in  individual  therapy  but  in  group  meetings, 
for  example,  in  staff  meetings,  in  rounds.  The  average  psychiatrist  does 
not  have  access  to  this  wealth  of  information;  the  average  resident  is 
exposed  to  it  for  a  year  or  two  and  then  goes  off,  into  private  practice." 

291 


And  the  man  in  private  practice  who  wants  to  do  a  clinical  study  of  some 
condition,  the  investigator  points  out,  has  to  work  with  very  few  people. 
Not  more  than  half-a-dozen  institutions  in  this  country,  says  Dr.  Lid;., 
are  able  to  undertake  the  intensive  and  long-term  clinical  research  needed 
if  we  are  to  make  sizable  advances  against  schizophrenia. 

Research  Grant,  MH  728 

Date  of  Interview    Mar.  22,  1965 

References. 

Fleck,  S.  Family  dynamics  and  origin  of  schizophrenia.  Psychosom,  Med.,  1960,22,5, 
Fleck,  S.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  Norton,  Nea,  &  Lidz,  T  Interaction  between  hospital  Mnff 

and  families.  Psychiatry,  1957,20,4. 
Fleck,  S  ,  Lidz,  T.,  &  Cornelison,  Alice.  Comparison  of  parent-child  relationships  of 

male  and  female  schizophrenic  patients,  Arch.  Gen.  Psychiat.,  1963,  8. 
Fleck,  S.,  Lidz,  T.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  Schafer,  Sarah,  &  Terry,  Dorothy.  The  inlra- 

familial    environment   of   the  schizophrenic  patient.  In  J.H.  Masserman  (lid,), 

Individual  and  familial  dynamics.  New  York:  Grime  &  Stratton,  1959. 
Lidz, T.  Schizophrenia  and  the  family.  Psychiatry,  1958,  21,1. 
Lidz,  T.  The  relevance  of  family  studies  to  psychoanalytic  theory./,  new.  incut.  Ms., 

1962,135,2. 
Lidz,  T.  The  family  and  human  adaptation.  New  York:  International  Universities 

Press,  1963. 
Lidz,  T.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  Fleck,  S.,  &  Terry,  Dorothy.  The  intrafamilial  environment 

of  the  schizophrenic  patient:  I.  The  father.  Psychiatry,  1957,  20,  4. 
Lidz,  T.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  Fleck,  S.,  &  Terry,  Dorothy.  The  intrafamilial  environment 

of  the  schizophrenic   patient:  II.   Marital  schizm  and   marital  skew.  A  trim.  J. 

Psychiat.,  1957,  114,3. 
Lidz,  T.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  Terry,  Dorothy,  &  Fleck,  S   Intrafamilial  environ  men  1  of 

the  schizophrenic  patient.   VI.  The  transmission  of  irrationality    A.M. A.  Arch, 

Neural  Psychiat.,  1958,  79. 
Lidz,  T.,  &  Fleck,  S.  Schizophrenia,  human  integration,  and  the  role  of  the  fiiimly.  In 

D.  Jackson,  (Ed. ),  Etiology  of  schizophrenia.  New  York:  Basic  Books,  1959. 
Lidz,  T.,  &  Fleck,  S.  Family  studies  and  a  thcoiy  of  schizophrenia.  Manuscript,  l%5, 
Lidz,  T.,  &  Fleck,  S.  The  mothers  of  schizophrenic  patients.  Manuscript,  1965. 
Lidz,  T,,  Fleck,  S.,  Alanen,  Y,,  £  Cornelison,  Alice.  Schizophrenic  patients  and  llieir 

siblings.  Psychiatry,  1963,26,  1. 
Lidz,  T.,  Fleck,  S.,  &  Cornelison,  Alice  R,  Schizophrenia  and  the  family.  New  York: 

International  Universities  Press,  1 966.  (Monograph  series  on  schizophrenia,  No.  1.) 
Lidz,  T.,  Fleck,  S.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  &  Terry,  Dorothy.  The  intrafamilial  environment 

of  the  schizophrenic  patient;  IV.  Parental  personalities  and  family  interaction. 

Amer.J.  Orthopsychiat.,  1958,28,4. 

Lidz,  T.,  Schafer,  Sarah,  Fleck,  S.,  Cornelison,  Alice,  &  Terry,  Dorothy.  Rgo  dif- 
ferentiation and  schizophrenic  symptom  formation  in  identical  twins.  J,  A  met. 

Psycftoanal,  Association,  1962,  10,  1. 
Lidz,  T.,  Wild,  Cynthia,   Schafer,  Sarah,  Rosman,  Bernice,  &  Fleck,  S,  Thought 

disorders  in  the  parents  of  schizophrenic  patients:  a  study  utilizing  the  object 

sorting  test.  /.  Psychiat.  Res.,  1962,  1. 
Rosman,  Bernice,  Wild,  Cynthia,  Ricci,  Judith,  Fleck,  S.,  &  Lidz,  T.  Thought  disorders 

in  the  parents  of  schizophrenic  patients:  a  further  study  utilizing  the  object  sorting 

test./.  Psychiat.  res,  1964,  2(3),  211-221. 
Wild,  Cynthia,  Singer,  Margaret,  Rosman,  Bernice,  Ricci,  Judith,  &  Lidz,  T.  Using  the 

object  sorting  test  on  parents  of  schizophrenic  patients.  Arch.  Gen.  Psychiat.,  1965, 

13(5),  471-476. 


292 


Investigator 

William  Goldfarb,  M.D.,  Ph.  D. 

Henry  Ittleson  Center  for  Child  Research 

New  York,  N.Y. 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 

Research  getting  at  some  of  the  basic  problems  of  childhood  schizo- 
phrenia-including causes  and  effective  methods  of  treatment-is  being 
conducted  in  a  mansion  high  above  the  Henry  Hudson  Parkway  in  the 
Riverdaie  section  of  New  York  City.  The  mansion  is  the  Henry  Ittleson 
Center  for  Child  Research,  named  for  the  late  banker.  Twenty-four  schizo- 
phrenic children,  from  6  to  1 1  years  old,  live  and  go  to  school  there. 
Twenty  others  come  on  weekdays  for  treatment  and  for  classes,  which  aie 
taught  by  specially  trained  teachers  of  the  public  school  system.  The 
director  is  Dr.  William  Goldfarb,  a  pioneer  in  coordinating  clinical  research 
and  the  treatment  of  disturbed  youngsters.  He  is  also  associate  clinical 
professor  of  psychiatry  at  Columbia  University. 

The  Center  is  making  a  careful  study  of  schizophrenic  children  over 
time,  Dr.  Goldfarb  explains.  It  wants  to  sec  what  changes  occur  not  only 
while  the  children  are  at  the  Center  but  also  after  they  leave;  it  hopes  to 
follow  them  into  adulthood.  This  is  believed  to  be  the  first  study  to  keep 
track  of  schizophrenic  children  during  the  process  of  growth  lather  than 
to  look  back  at  them  after  years  of  treatment. 

The  first  findings  of  this  research  program,  which  began  a  dozen  years 
ago,  are  summarized  here;  some  are  presented  in  more  detail  further  on. 
They  are  based  on  observations  of  children  from  intact  families  only, 
because  the  investigators  are  especially  interested  in  how  the  charac- 
teristics of  the  parents,  and  the  ways  in  which  parents  relate  to  each  other 
and  the  children  can  affect  the  development  of  mental  illness.  The 
children  studied  come  from  all  socioeconomic  levels. 


Major  Findings  and  Conclusions 

1.  The  schizophrenic  children  under  study  fall  in  to  two  main  groups, 
labeled  by  the  investigators  as  organic  and  nonorganic.  Children  classified 
as  .organic  are  considered  to  have  brain  damage,  but  this  is  revealed  only 
by  a  close  examination.  Their  neurological  signs  are  usually  "soft," 
meaning  equivocal.  (Psychotic  youngsters  with  obvious  neurological 
defects  are  excluded  and  referred  to  neurological  services.)  Children 


classified  as  nonorganic  have  no  sign  of  even  slight  physiological  impair- 
ment. Of  the  1 29  children  studied  so  far,  about  two-thirds  have  been 
placed  in  the  organic  group. 

2.  The  families  of  the  organic  or  brain-damaged  children  are  of  more 
nearly  normal  adequacy  than  the  families  of  the  other  schizophrenics.  An 
early  study   found   that  almost  half  of  the   nonorganic  schizophrenic 
children  had  mothers  who  were  schizophrenic,  too.  A  new  study  is  it) 
process. 

3.  The  symptoms  of  the  organic  child  do  not  spring  directly  fiom  liis 
physiological  impairment  but  from  family-child  inteiactions  set  in  motion 
when  he  fails  to  respond  quite  normally  to  his  environment.  The  parents* 
response  to  this  failure  is  to  stimulate  him  too  much  or  too  little,  general- 
ly the  former,  or  to  confuse  Mm.  His  response  stamps  him  more  than  ever 
as  different.  The  cycle  continues.  The  product  is  a  child  so  confused  about 
himself  and  the  world  that  he  withdraws  more  or  less  completely  uml 
exhibits  other  characteristics  of  the  childhood  schizophrenic. 

4.  The  symptoms  of  the  nonorganic,  or  apparently  undamaged,  child 
may  have  been  influenced  by  some  undiscovered  defect.  But  they  appear 
to  be  completely  explainable  as  maladaptive  reactions  to  an  abnormal 
environment.  This   environment,  which  has  been  shaped  by   "parental 
perplexity"   or   "parental  paralysis,"  is  marked   by   indecisiveness,  in- 
sensitivity  to  the   child's  needs,  bewilderment  in  the  face  of  unusual 
behavior,  and  inability  to  direct  the  child.  The  end,  again,  is  a  confused 
and  panic-stricken  child  whose  bizarre  behavior,  like  that  of  the  organic 
youngster,  is  a  means  of  coping  with  his  distress. 

5.  Under  residential  treatment,  both  groups  of  schizophrenic  children 
improve  clinically,  acquire  learning  skills,  and  rate  higher  on  a  standard 
test  of  intelligence.  About  three-fourths  return  to  the  community.  Bui 
there  is  a  decided  difference  in  their  response  to  the  Center's  schooling.  A 
study  of  37  children  in  residence  during  a  recent  3-year  period  found  that 
all  those  in  the  nonorganic  group  eventually  gave  at  least  a  normal  per- 
formance in  reading  and  arithmetic.  In  the  organic  group,  most  of  the 
children  improved  but  reached  a  peak  that  was  lower  than  normal  and 
then  leveled  off.  In  sum,  as  emotional  disabilities  were  alleviated,  the 
children  became  free  to  use  their  potentiality  for  learning,  which  was  at 
least  normal  in  the  nonorganic  group  but  below  normal  in  the  organic. 

6.  For  some  schizophrenic  children,  the  Center's  experimental  day 
treatment  program  seems  to  be  just  as  effective  as  its  residential  program. 
These  are  the  children  in  the  organic  group.  The  nonorganic  children  in 
the  day  program  do  not  do  nearly  so  well  as  those  who  live  at  the  Center. 
Presumably    this   is  because  the  former  return  every   afternoon  to  a 
pathogenic  family  environment. 

Dr.  Goldfarb  points  out  that  the  division  of  schizophrenic  children  into 
two  broad  groups  is  not  yet  generally  accepted,  though  evidence  of 
organic  impairment  in  some  child  patients  has  been  reported  by  half  n 
dozen  other  investigators  as  well. 


One  big  impediment  to  geneial  agreement  on  the  causes  and  nature  of 
childhood  schizophrenia,  Dr.  Goldfarb  believes,  is  that  different  investi- 
gators use  different  means  of  evaluating  their  patients  and  different  labels 
for  describing  the  results.  To  clarify  the  Center's  research  techniques  and 
findings,  Dr.  Goldfarb  and  his  associates  are  making  a  moving  picture 
showing  some  of  the  children  under  study  and  in  each  case  the  reasons  for 
classifying  the  child  neurologically  as  clearly  organic,  equivocally  organic, 
or  clearly  nonorganic. 

Beyond  offering  information  on  the  causes  of  childhood  schizophrenia, 
the  Centei's  findings  have  important  implications  for  treatment.  They 
strongly  indicate,  for  example,  that  the  majority  of  schizophrenic  children 
are  educable.  (Most  of  the  children  in  the  learning  study  had  arrived  at  the 
Center  without  educational  accomplishments;  almost  all  had  been 
considered  unmanageable  by  the  schools  in  the  community— and  most  of 
them  uneducable  as  well.)  The  study  also  suggests  that  day  programs  for 
schizophrenic  children  can  be  very  successful,  particularly  if  limited  to 
children  like  those  found  by  the  Ittleson  Center  to  show  signs  of  neuro- 
logical impairment.  Day  programs  are  far  less  expensive  than  residential 
programs.  The  aveiagc  annual  cost  for  a  child  in  residence  at  the  Center  is 
about  $8,500  (the  bulk  of  which  is  met  by  city,  State,  and  private  contri- 
butions, these  last  funneled  through  the  Jewish  Board  of  Guardians,  a 
charitable  organization  under  whose  auspices  the  Center  was  chartered). 
In  contrast,  the  average  annual  cost  for  a  child  in  the  day  program  is 
approximately  $4,500. 

The  Ittleson  Center  has  undertaken  its  long-term  study  not  merely  to 
learn  what  happens  to  childhood  schizophrenics,  but  also  to  test  the 
effects  of  certain  treatment  procedures.  Among  the  questions  it  hopes  to 
answer  are:  (1)  Will  treatment  directed  at  correcting  a  specific  problem  of 
a  schizophrenic  child-far  example,  lack  of  self-awareness-make  for 
overall  improvement?  and  (2)  Will  work  to  improve  communications 
within  the  family -which  are  judged  to  be  abnormal,  particularly  in  the 
nonorganic  cases— result  in  improvement  in  the  child? 

Dr.  Goldfarb  hopes  also  to  study  a  group  of  children  who  have  neuro- 
logical defects  but  are  emotionally  normal.  The  reason  they  have  not 
become  schizophrenic,  he  believes,  lies  in  the  way  they  have  been  handled 
at  home.  Detailed  information  about  this  would  presumably  help 
substantiate  the  Center's  findings  about  the  causes  of  childhood  schizo- 
phrenia and  increase  our  ability  to  take  preventive  measures. 


Three  Principal  Defects  of  Schizophrenic  Children 

The  children  at  the  Henry  Ittleson  Center  differ  widely  in  capabilities, 
behavior,  and,  the  investigators  believe,  in  the  causes  of  their  disorder.  But 
they  are  alike  in  displaying  extreme  emotional  reactions  that  combine 
fear,  anger,  and  disorganized  motor  response.  These  reactions-which  the 
investigators  consider  to  be  similar  to  the  fear  reactions  of  very  early 
childhood -seem  to  spring  from  the  children's  overwhelming  feelings  of 

295 


strangeness  about  themselves  and  their  world.  And  these  feelings  are 
believed  to  be  rooted  in  the  three  major  defects  that  the  investigators, 
originally  on  the  basis  of  clinical  observation  alone,  found  common  to  all 
these  children. 

The  first  defect  lies  in  the  processing  of  perceptual  information.  The 
children  have  rheir  senses  but  do  not  make  normal  use  of  them,  hi 
particular,  they  pay  little  attention  to  what  they  see  and  hear.  Some  of 
them  when  they  come  to  the  Center  appear  to  be  deaf. 

The  second  big  trouble  is  a  deficiency  in  the  child's  awaiencss  of  self -• 
the  absence  of  a  sure  sense  of  a  unified  and  intact  body.  Most  of  usgiow 
up  and  think  we  have  always  had  that  sense,  Dr.  Golclfarb  lemnrks; 
actually  we  achieved  it.  This  defect  is  manifested  in  a  variety  of  ways.  One 
child  was  found  talking  to  her  hands  as  if  they  were  beings  quite  in- 
dependent of  herself.  Another  asked  if  her  hands  would  fall  off.  Children 
cutting  paper  have  to  be  watched,  for  several  have  cut  into  their  fingeis, 
unaware  that  their  fingers  are  part  of  themselves-and  apparently  unaware, 
too,  that  the  pain  of  a  cut  finger  results  from  an  action  by  themselves. 
One  child  asked  to  have  her  head  opened  because  there  were  men  running 
around  in  it;  a  dental  examination  found  a  badly  decayed  tooth.  Another 
child,  walking  with  a  counselor,  asked:  "Is  this  a  long  walk?  Am  I  tiicd? 
Do  my  feet  hurt?  Will  my  feet  hurt?" 

Some  of  the  children  are  not  sure  whether  they  are  boys  or  girls,  or  of 
their  physiological  needs.  They  become  agitated  when  hungry  or  when 
under  pressure  to  relieve  bladder  or  bowels.  But  they  have  to  be  told  to 
eat  or  to  go  the  toilet;  they  also  have  to  be  told  to  stop  eating  or  to  leave 
the  toilet,  for  they  have  no  sense  of  when  their  needs  have  been  met.  Such 
children,  unable  to  find  gratification  even  in  satisfying  hunger,  arc  un- 
common, but  it  is  characteristic  of  childhood  schizophrenics,  Dr.  Goldfarb 
notes,  to  be  confused  about  pleasure  as  well  as  pain  and  to  find  trouble 
directing  themselves  into  pleasure-giving  activities. 

The  principal  defects  noted  so  far-in  self-awareness  and  in  the  Ability 
to  take  in  and  organize  what  the  senses  perceive-lead  to  a  confusion  jilso 
about  space  and  time.  To  allay  the  resulting  anxiety,  schizophrenic  chil- 
dren use  a  variety  of  mechanisms,  including  withdrawal,  a  seeking  for 
sameness,  and  a  compulsive  over-concern  with  time  and  place,  marked  by 
endless  questions  about  where  something  is  or  when  some  daily  event  is 
going  to  happen. 

The  third  major  characteristic  of  the  schizophrenic  children  studied  is  a 
difficulty  in  communicating  with  people.  Like  the  other  characteristics 
noted,  it  occurs  universally  but  in  many  degrees  and  forms,  ranging  from 
mutism  to  talking  in  such  a  way  that  the  child  cannot  be  fully  understood. 


Two  Groups  of  Schizophrenics 

Compared  With  Normal  Children 

Long  observation  led  Dr,  Goldfarb  and  the  <?ther  staff  psychiatrists  lo 
conclude   that  many  of  the  schizophrenic  children -surprisingly  many, 

296 


they  thought-had  inherent  defects.  Their  view  was  supported  by  a  neurol- 
ogist who  was  not  on  the  Center's  staff  and  who  did  not  know  how  the 
children  had  been  tentatively  classified.  He  found  that  a  majority  of  the 
first  26  children  intensively  studied  gave  evidence  of  neurologic  impair- 
ment—54  percent  in  their  examination,  58  percent  in  their  history,  65 
percent  in  either  examination  or  history.  The  neurologist  and  the  psychia- 
trist saw  eye  to  eye  8  times  out  of  10. 

The  organic  and  nonorganic  groups  were  then  compared  with  a  group 
of  normal  children  and  with  each  other  on  the  basis  of  a  number  of 
neurological  and  psychological  tests.  The  findings  provide  important  infor- 
mation about  the  underlying  reasons  for  the  principal  defects  common  to 
childhood  schizophrenics.  They  also  show,  the  researchers  believe,  that 
certain  behavioral  variations  usually  consideied  characteristic  of  childhood 
schizophrenics  in  general  are,  in  fact,  most  characteristic  of  the  group 
designated  by  these  investigators  as  organic. 

All  three  groups  of  children  were  found  to  have  the  same  average  sen- 
sory acuity— in  sight,  hearing,  and  touch.  But  marked  differences  appeared 
in  the  children's  responses  to  perceptual  tests.  Some  of  these  tests  dealt 
with  the  ability  to  differentiate  body  cues.  Esthesiometer  tests,  for  ex- 
ample, measured  the  child's  ability  to  discriminate  two  tactile  stimuli 
presented  simultaneously;  the  finger-location  test  determined  his  ability  to 
localize  the  fingers  of  his  hand  (when  these,  out  of  his  sight,  were  touched 
one  by  one).  These  abilities,  Dr.  Goldfarb  points  out,  help  make  it  possi- 
ble for  a  person  to  have  a  clear  image  of  his  body.  Other  tests  measured 
the  ability  to  discriminate  a  picture  from  its  background  and  the  ability  to 
form  a  picture  from  fragmented  parts. 

In  all  these  tests  the  schizophrenic  children  were  markedly  inferior  to 
the  normals,  and  in  all  except  the  figure-discrimination  test  the  organic 
group  children  were  markedly  inferior  to  the  nonorganic.  Evidently,  says 
Dr.  Goldfarb,  the  schizophrenic  youngster  takes  in  messages  from  the 
environment  but  doesn't  handle  them  properly -doesn't  make  normal  pat- 
terns of  them  or  respond  to  them  normally.  So  he  has  no  clear  conscious- 
ness either  of  himself  or  of  his  environment. 

The  results  of  the  delayed  auditory  feedback  test  are  taken  to  support 
that  conclusion.  In  this  test  a  child's  voice  was  returned  to  him  after  a 
delay  of  0.1 6  second.  When  a  normal  child  heard  his  voice  after  the  delay, 
his  speech  became  distinctly  different  but  he  recognized  his  voice  and 
showed  no  signs  of  confusion.  When  the  schizophrenic  child— of  either 
group— heard  his  voice,  his  speech  did  not  change  but  he  did  not  recognize 
it  as  his  own.  Further,  he  acted  as  though  confused,  restless,  and  under 
tension.  The  investigator  concludes  that  the  schizophrenic  youngster, 
since  he  did  not  change  his  speech,  was  speaking  without  hearing  or,  more 
probably,  without  listening.  This  finding  supports  clinical  observations 
that  the  schizophrenic  child  tends  to  exclude  hearing  as  a  means  of  learn- 
ing about  and  dealing  with  the  world  around  him.  The  investigator  also 
concludes  that  the  schizophrenic's  inability  to  recognize  his  own  voice 
shows  a  limitation  in  his  awareness  of  himself.  The  results  of  this  limita- 
tion are  to  be  seen  in  his  confusion  and  restlessness,  often  amounting  to 

297 


panic,    under   the   conditions  of  the  experiment,  and  his  feelings  of 
strangeness,  fear,  and  rage  in  everyday  life. 

Persons  with  perceptual  impairments  of  the  type  shown  by  the  schizo- 
phrenic children  at  the  Ittleson  Center,  Dr.  Goldfarb  observes,  might  be 
expected  to  have  impairments  also  in  the  higher  cognitive  processes 
involved  in  conceptualization.  The  tests  bear  out  that  expectation.  They 
show  that  the  schizophrenic  child  (a)  finds  it  difficult  to  differentiate 
right  and  left;  (b)  is  poorly  oriented  for  time,  place,  and  person;  (c)  has 
trouble  grouping  objects  by  foim  and  color;  and  (d)  represents  the  human 
body  far  moie  primitively  than  normal  children.  In  all  the  tests  leading  to 
these  results,  the  organic  group  was  significantly  worse  than  the  non- 
organic. 

To  measure  overall  intellectual  functioning,  the  investigators  used  the 
Wechsler  Intelligence  Scale  for  Children.  The  schizophrenic  children 
proved  to  be  inferior  to  the  normal  children  both  in  total  IQand  in  each 
of  its  components-verbal  and  performance  IQ.  More  than  half  of  the 
schizophrenic  children  but  none  of  the  others  had  IQ's  in  the  retarded 
range-under  75.  More  than  90  percent  of  the  normal  children  but  only  23 
percent  of  the  schizophrenics  snowed  at  least  average  capacity— IQ's  above 
90.  Marked  differences  appeared  between  the  two  groups  of  Ittleson 
Center  children.  The  average  IQ  of  the  nonorganic  schizophrenics  was  92; 
of  the  organic,  62  (as  compared  with  109  for  the  normal  children). 

Tests  of  motor  coordination  and  locomotor  balance  showed  similar 
variations.  On  the  Lincoln-Oseretsky  test,  which  provides  36  motor  tasks, 
the  normal  children's  average  score  was  70.1  and  the  schizophrenic  chil- 
dren's 27.9,  but  the  nonorganic  group  averaged  48;  the  organic  only  1 7.2. 
On  the  railwalking  test,  the  average  scores  were  60.2  for  the  normal  chil- 
dien  and  23.7  for  the  schizophrenics,  but  the  nonorganic  group  scored 
43,8;  the  organic,  only  1 3. 

Another  differentiating  measure  was  the  whirling  test,  in  which  the 
child  stands  erect,  eyes  closed  and  arms  extended  parallel  to  each  other, 
and  the  examiner  applies  varying  degrees  of  pressure  to  turn  the  child's 
head,  without  discomfort.  In  general  contrast  to  the  nonorganic  schizo- 
phrenic, the  organic  schizophrenic  tended  to  whirl-that  is,  to  turn  his 
whole  body,  as  long  as  pressure  was  applied,  in  the  direction  his  head  was 
turned.  Such  a  response,  not  shown  by  children  in  the  normal  group,  is 
considered  to  indicate  immature  development  of  the  central  nervous  sys- 
tem. 

The  investigators  were  especially  interested  in  the  results  of  the 
auditory  startle  test.  When  normal  children,  wearing  earphones,  were 
subjected  to  a  sudden  loud  tone,  they  showed  that  they  were  aware  of  it 
but  not  distressed  by  it.  The  schizophrenic  children  gave  a  wide  range  of 
reactions.  At  one  extreme,  eight  children  showed  no  evidence  of  hearing 
the  tone  at  all.  At  the  other  extreme,  six  children  showed  extreme  dis- 
comfort. With  one  exception,  all  these  extreme  cases  were  in  the  organic 
group. 

Dr.  Goldfarb  reports  that  children  at  the  Center  who  give  no  evidence 
of  hearing  speech  or  other  sounds  on  one  occasion  may  react  with 

298 


frenzied  distress  on  others,  and  that  such  fluctuation  between  pseudodeaf- 
ness  and  distress  may  be  seen  in  the  same  child  on  the  same  day.  The 
auditory  startle  test,  he  believes,  indicates  that  the  most  marked  forms  of 
hypei  sensitivity  and  sensory  avoidance  are  linked  with  organicity  rather 
than  nonorgamcity.  But  some  measure  of  sensory  avoidance  does,  he 
emphasizes,  frequently  occur  among  nonorganic  schizophrenics. 

Measurement  of  speech  and  voice  characteristics,  through  techniques 
developed  at  the  Center,  showed  distinct  differences  from  one  schizo- 
phrenic child  to  another,  but  not  between  the  two  groups.  All  the  schizo- 
phrenics had  deviances  in  such  matters  as  volume,  pitch,  fluency,  stress, 
intonation,  and  inflection-characteristics  that  determine  a  person's  ability 
to  get  across  meanings  beyond  those  in  the  words  themselves.  Ordinarily 
the  culture's  standards  for  these  characteristics  are  learned  very  early  in 
life.  Since  the  schizophrenics  had  learned  them  only  partly  if  at  all,  these 
children  had  a  way  of  speaking  that  isolated  them  still  further  from 
normal  activities.  Typically  the  voice  was  so  inexpressive  that  even  when  a 
child's  words  could  be  understood,  the  person  talking  to  him  felt  shut  out 
and  unable  to  get  through. 

To  sum  up,  children  in  both  the  organic  and  the  nonorganic  groups 
were  found  to  be  measurably  different  from  normal  children  in  a  number 
of  tests  of  perception,  cognition,  and  psychomotor  functions.  In  certain 
cases-notably  the  delayed  auditory  feedback  test  and  the  measures  of 
communicative  ability— the  children  in  the  nonoiganic  group  were  found 
to  deviate  as  greatly  from  the  normal  as  those  in  the  organic  group.  In 
general,  though,  the  investigators  believe,  the  results  support  the  conclu- 
sion that  childhood  schizophrenics  can  be  divided  into  two  general 
clusters— indeed,  that  such  a  division  is  necessary  to  an  understanding  of 
childhood  schizophrenia, 

"What  is  currently  called  schizophrenia,"  Dr.  Goldfarb  declares,  "is  a 
hodgepodge  population  that  needs  subclassification.  In  mental  deficiency 
it  has  become  increasingly  possible  to  separate  out  subpopulations  on  the 
basis  of  cause,  such  as  phenylketonuria  or  severe  maternal  deprivation. 
This  is  likely  to  be  the  history  of  schizophrenia."  But  in  working  out  a 
subclassification,  he  emphasizes,  the  relationship  between  the  child  and 
his  family  has  to  be  thoroughly  investigated  even  in  those  cases  where  an 
organic  defect  is  apparent. 

Reproductive  Complications 

The  comparison  of  schizophrenic  and  normal  children  has  been  carried 
a  step  farther  through  a  study  of  their  prenatal  and  perinatal  histories-the 
record  of  events  before  and  during  birth  and  shortly  afterward.  In  this 
phase  of  the  research,  the  investigators  studied  three  groups:  (a)  29  chil- 
dren in  residential  treatment  at  the  Center;  (b)  39  brothers  and  sisters  of 
these  children;  (c)  34  public  school  children  comparable  to  the  schizo- 
phrenic children  in  ages  and  backgrounds. 

299 


The  comparison  was  made  on  the  basis  of  a  "reproductive  complica- 
tions lecord"  listing  83  conditions  that  had  been  used  by  other  investi- 
gators in  studying  factors  associated  with  cerebral  palsy,  mental  retarda- 
tion, and  childhood  behavior  disorders.  Many  of  these  conditions  arc 
suspected  rather  than  known  to  be  related  to  fetal  damage.  A  few  of  (he 
items  are  listed  below.  Those  in  the  first  three  groups  were  scored  phism\ 
considered  presumptive  evidence  of  reproductive  complications.  Items  in 
the  fourth  group,  The  Newborn,  were  scored  either  plus  or  double  plus, 
generally  depending  upon  the  severity  of  the  condition;  a  double  pins 
rating  was  considered  highly  significant  evidence  of  reproductive  compli- 
cation. 

Historical  (+) 

Mother's  age  over  45 

Abdominal  or  pelvic  X-ray  of  either  parent 

Accidental  conception  despite  using  spermicidal  contraceptive  jelly 

Pregnancy  O) 

Total  gestation  period  less  than  8  months 

Smoked  20  or  more  cigarettes  daily 

Excessive  nausea 

Severe  emotional  shock 

Any  infection 

Endocrine  disorder 

Unusual  medication 
Delivery  (+) 

False  labor 

First  stage  of  labor  more  than  24  hours 

Face,  brow,  breech,  or  transverse  presentation 

Midforceps,  high  forceps 

Precipitous  delivery 

Placenta  previa 

Baby  held  back 
The  Newborn  (+  or  ++) 

Cyanosis  +  or  ++ 

Weak  cry  + 

Absent  cry  ++ 

Injuries  +  or -H- 

Jaundice  +  or  ++ 

Vomiting  +  or  -H- 

Convulsions  -H- 

Abnormal  reflexes  -H- 

When  information  from  all  sources-mothers,  attending  physicians,  and 
hospital  records-was  used,  the  schizophrenic  children  were  found  to  have 
had  more  than  half  again  as  many  reproductive  complications  on  the 
average  as  their  brothers  and  sisters  and  the  public  school  children.  The 
difference  in  the  number  of  complications,  both  those  rated  more  signifi- 
cant and  those  rated  less  significant,  was  particularly  marked  in  the  case  of 
the  boys. 

One  of  the  problems  in  a  study  like  this,  the  investigators  point  out,  is 
that  data  from  the  mothers  of  severely  disordered  children  "are  inevitably 
selected  and  colored  by  such  repressive  or  distracting  influences  as  guilt, 
illusory  wish  fulfillment,  or  defensive  denial."  So  the  investigators  place 

300 


more  weight  on  the  analysis  of  hospital  data  which  had  been  recorded  at 
the  time  of  observation  and  without  any  knowledge  of  how  the  infant 
would  turn  out.  This  analysis  shows  that  the  average  number  of  repro- 
ductive complications  is  significantly  greater  for  the  schizophrenic  boys 
than  for  their  brothers  or  for  the  boys  in  the  public  school  group. 
Between  the  schizophrenic  girls  and  the  girls  in  the  other  groups,  however, 
there  is  no  significant  difference. 

The  sample  was  small,  and  the  Ittleson  Center's  children  may  not  be 
representative  of  all  childhood  schizophrenics.  Nevertheless,  the  investi- 
gators say,  the  finding  apparently  ties  in  with  earlier  findings  by  other 
researchers  that  (a)  more  boys  than  girls  have  childhood  schizophrenia  and 
(6)  more  boys  than  girls  are  affected  by  disorders  such  as  cerebral  palsy, 
where  brain  damage  is  an  undisputed  factor,  and  by  mental  retardation, 
where  brain  damage  frequently  has  a  significant  role.  The  investigators 
note  also  that  the  ratio  of  boys  to  girls  at  the  Center  is  about  three  times 
as  high  in  the  organic  group  as  in  the  nonorganic. 

All  in  all,  this  research  team  is  inclined  to  believe  that: 

•  Cerebral   dysfunction   as  a  result  of  reproductive  trauma   is  more 
typical  of  the  boy  than  the  girl  schizophrenic. 

•  The  higher  proportion  of  boys  among  schizophrenic  children  reflects 
the   importance   of  brain  damage  as  a  contributing  factor  to  the 
adaptive  disturbances  in  these  children. 

Dr.  Goldfarb  emphasizes  that  brain  damage,  from  whatever  cause,  need 
not  lead  to  a  behavior  disorder— and  that  even  when  it  does,  the  disorder 
may  well  not  be  childhood  schizophrenia.  Further,  childhood  schizo- 
phrenia can  occur  even  when  the  brain  seems  organically  normal.  Evident- 
ly the  experience  of  the  child  in  his  family  relationships  is  also  important. 


The  Families  of  Schizophrenic  Children 

Before  a  child  is  admitted  to  the  Ittleson  Center  and  then  again  when 
he  is  discharged,  a  trained  observer  pays  a  long  visit  to  the  child's  family 
and  notes  the  interactions  between  mother  and  father,  both  as  wife  and 
husband  and  as  parents;  between  parents  and  children;  among  the 
children;  and  among  all  the  family  members.  The  observer  rates  the  family 
on  46  aspects  of  its  behavior,  including  the  spontaneity  of  interactions, 
the  methods  used  by  the  parents  to  exercise  control,  and  the  ways  in 
which  the  children's  needs  are  met.  In  each  case  a  high  score  means  that 
the  members  are  acting  in  a  way  that  reflects  and  enhances  the  mental 
health  of  the  group.  The  sum  of  the  ratings  is  called  the  family  adequacy 
score. 

The  research  group  has  used  this  technique  to  compare  the  families  of 
22  children  at  the  Center  with  those  of  22  normal  children.  The  families 
of  the  schizophrenic  children  in  the  nonorganic  group  were  found  to  be 
the  least  adequate,  by  far.  On  the  average  they  scored  158  out  of  a 
possible  300.  The  families  of  the  other  schizophrenic  children  scored  206, 

301 


25  such  visits  has  been  concerned  principally  with  the  communicnUoual 
behavior  of  the  mother. 

Some  of  the  women  made  little  effort  to  talk  with  their  children 
Others  talked  a  good  deal,  but  in  a  way  that  was  obviously  confusing.  One 
mother,  for  example,  kept  saying  to  her  6-year-old  boy,  "How  nre  you? 
How  are  you  doing?"  at  a  time  when  he  was  crying  and  saying  that  he 
wanted  to  go  home.  At  another  point,  she  asked,  "What  did  you  have  for 
lunch,  Jimmy -ham?"  He  answered,  "Fish."  Then  she  said,  "Jimmy,  what 
did  you  have  for  lunch,  huh?"  and  he  said  "Fish."  Then  she  said  again, 
"Tell  me,  what  did  you  have  for  lunch,  sweetheart?"  He  stood  in  front  of 
her  and  looked  at  her  and  said,  "Fish."  By  laying  down  a  barrage  of 
topics,  this  mother,  like  some  others,  discouraged  the  child  from  com- 
municating his  own  feelings. 

When  the  mother  of  Mary -a  child  with  a  great  deal  of  anxiety  about 
her  body-greeted  her,  Mary  said,  "Finger,"  and  smiled  and  motioned  to 
her  finger.  The  mother  said,  "What's  that?"  Mary  replied,  "Finger,"  and 
smiled  broadly.  The  mother  said,  "What  is  it?"  Mary  answered,  "1  love 
finger."  The  mother  made  no  verbal  response.  This  interchange,  the1  in- 
vestigators point  out,  illustrates  both  the  child's  deviant  means  of  com- 
municating her  need  for  affection  and  the  mothei's  failure  to  do  anything 
to  counteract  this  deviancy  or  to  meet  the  need.  Earlier,  Mary's  mother 
had  told  the  Center  that  she  and  her  husband  both  had  been  afniid  to 
touch  the  child  as  a  baby.  "Both  of  us  got  up  in  the  night,  and  one  gave 
her  the  bottle  and  one  held  the  baby,"  the  mother  said.  "I  put  a  hand- 
kerchief in  his  hand  to  hold  the  baby's  head  because  I  was  afraid  thnl  if  he 
would  touch  her  with  his  bare  hands,  her  head  would  dilapidate." 

A  common  error  reducing  communicational  clarity  was  found  to  be 
discordance  between  the  mother's  expression  and  the  child's  level  of  com- 
prehension. For  example,  a  10-year-old  boy  greeted  his  mother  with  a 
French  word,  combien.  (The  investigators  learned  after  the  visit  that  Ihis 
apparently  meaningless  word  was  an  error  on  his  part.  He  had  actually  had 
a  French  lesson  that  day  and  had  meant  to  say  "Comment  allez-voux?"in 
an  effort  to  win  his  mother's  praise.)  His  mother  did  not  express  her 
puzzlement  about  combien,  nor  did  she  insist  on  an  explanation  so  thill  he 
would  sound  less  incoherent.  And  she  missed  entirely  his  natural  dcsiic  for 
praise.  She  said,  "Are  you  speaking  French  already?"  and  then,  "You 
couldn't  have  learned  it  all  in  a  couple  of  days.  There  must  still  be  sonic 
left  to  do."  This  remark  distressed  him  because  he  thought  she  was  saying 
he  had  not  studied  his  lessons,  whereas  she  was  actually  using  an  adult 
variety  of  sarcastic  humor.  He  said,  "I  did  really  my  studies."  Eight 
minutes  later,  he  persisted,  "Mommy,  how  come  you  said  1  couldn't  learn 
it  all  yet?" 

Such  errors  occur  also  in  interchanges  between  mothers  and  normal 
children,  the  investigator  notes,  but  tend  to  be  much  milder.  This  mother, 
Jimmy's  mother,  and  a  number  of  others  displayed  what  the  investigators 
describe  as  tmpervtousness:  they  failed  to  recognize,  or  at  least  to 
acknowledge,  a  child's  feelings  and  meanings. 

304 


Each  mother  was  given  a  clarity  score,  based  on  an  analysis  of  the 
transcript  of  the  first  few  minutes  of  the  visit.  In  general,  the  mothers  of 
the  16  children  classified  as  organic  scored  higher  than  the  others.  This 
findings  supports  the  hypothesis,  Dr.  Gold  far  b  notes,  that  the  roots  of 
childhood  schizophrenia,  in  the  cases  where  there  is  no  neurological 
impairment,  lie  in  parental  failure.  The  normal  mother  spends  much  time 
explaining  novel  experiences  to  a  child  so  that  he  can  giasp  them  and 
relate  them  to  previously  achieved  understandings.  She  stimulates  and 
guides  his  psychological  development,  encouraging  some  of  his  responses 
and  discouraging  those  that  are  not  acceptable.  By  voice  and  gesture  she 
stimulates  an  interest  in  acoustic  and  visual  experience.  She  encourages 
the  child  to  be  attentive  to  her  communicational  clues,  and  she  is  attentive 
to  his.  The  communicational  failures  observed  during  the  mother-child 
visits  at  the  Center  suggest  to  the  investigators  that  many  of  the  mothers, 
particularly  those  whose  children  are  neurologically  sound,  did  not 
successfully  cairy  out  such  aspects  of  the  maternal  role. 

Dr.  Goldfarb  and  his  associates  recognize  that  the  verbal  interactions  in 
this  study  may  have  been  influenced  not  only  by  the  characteristics  of  the 
mothers  but  also  by  the  setting  in  which  the  interactions  took  place— a 
visit  to  a  child  in  an  institution.  To  obtain  comparative  data,  the  re- 
searchers have  now  arranged  to  study  mothers  who  visit  their  children  in 
an  orthopedic  hospital. 


Treating  the  Child  in  a  Residential  Center 

As  the  result  of  its  clinical  observations,  its  tests,  and  its  study  of  the 
families,  the  Center  has  developed  a  treatment  program  it  describes  as 
corrective  socialization. 

In  one  way  or  another,  Dr.  Goldfarb  notes,  the  child's  home  environ- 
ment failed  to  supply  what  the  child  needed  in  order  to  perceive  and 
understand  reality— that  is,  to  get  a  true  picture  of  events  outside  of 
himself  and  inside  of  himself— and  to  act  accordingly.  The  failure  occurred 
in  many  cases  because  the  children  presented  a  special  challenge  to  which 
the  parents  did  not  react  appropriately.  In  the  other  cases  the  failure 
resulted  from  a  high  degree  of  parental  confusion  and  paralysis.  Whatever 
the  primary  cause  of  the  failure,  the  consequence  was  "a  calamitous  and 
unvarying  state  of  strangeness," 

Dr.  Goldfarb  believes  that  this  feeling  of  strangeness  explains  the  states 
of  panic  often  noted  among  schizophrenic  children.  "The  persistent  and 
overburdening  sense  of  strangeness  makes  it  difficult  for  the  child  to  cope 
with  what  is  unfamiliar,"  he  says,  "and  restricts  the  likelihood  of  the  kind 
of  satisfying  complacency  which  accompanies  the  perception  of  what  is 
familiar."  He  and  his  associates  call  the  panic  states  "primordial"  because 
they  are  considered  to  represent  elemental  reactions  to  strangeness  (in 
contrast  to  the  panic  that  can  arise  from  unconscious  conflict). 

The  Center's  therapeutic  approach  has  these  general  goals:  To  help  the 
schizophrenic  child  orient  himself  to  the  real  world  and  to  improve  his 

305 


ability  to  recognize  himself  as  an  individual  capable  of  self-direction -t 
realize,  for  example,  that  this  is  his  hand  and  that  he  can  throw  a  ball  wit 
it  if  he  wants  to.  Toward  the  achievement  of  these  goals,  the  staff  of  cigh 
psychiatrists,  eight  teachers,  five  social  workeis,  four  psychologists,  tw 
nurses,  and  nine  child  care  workers  01  counselors-including  member 
assigned  to  the  day  program-tries  in  many  ways  to  telieve  the  child' 
strangeness  and  anxiety  and  to  enlarge  his  range  of  gratification  am 
pleasure. 

The  children  are  given  a  carefully  structured  world,  having  boundari? 
in  space  and  time  that  they  can  come  to  recognize  and  hold  onto.  TJn 
Center's  large  grounds  have  been  marked  off  into  small  areas  havhij 
clearly  defined  functions-a  garden  area,  a  playground  area,  a  bicycle  area 
(Before  the  grounds  were  divided,  the  children  wandered  over  them 
aimlessly  and  anxiously.)  Inside,  too,  the  space  is  carefully  delineated  in 
terms  of  the  function  it  serves,  and  the  children  are  encouraged  to  move 
freely  within  the  bounds  appropriate  to  the  current  activity.  At  bedtime, 
for  example,  a  child  may  move  about  his  group's  bedroom,  bathroom, and 
playroom.  At  schooltime,  he  may  move  about  the  classioom.  Even  the 
most  jegressed  child,  Dr.  Goldfarb  reports,  quickly  learns  the  spatial 
boundaiies  for  any  given  activity. 

The  children  learn,  too,  that  there  are  set  times  for  specific  activities 
each  day,  each  week,  and  each  month.  Acceptable  variants  of  behavior 
with  respect  to  time  are  taught.  For  example,  pajamas  are  worn  to  bed  at 
night,  clothes  must  be  worn  during  the  day,  overclothes  are  worn  outside 
in  winter. 

In  all  talk  with  the  children  regarding  space  and  time,  the  staff  tries  to 
be  completely  clear.  The  child  is  told  again  and  again  what  he  is  going  (o 
do  and  where  and  when.  He  is  offered  choices,  but  these  are  simple-die 
choice,  for  instance,  between  playing  in  the  bedroom  or  in  the  playroom. 
Activities,  and  communications  about  activities,  are  linked  as  often  as 
possible  to  rewards;  the  child  is  told,  for  example,  "In  the  morning  we 
dress  and  then  go  down  for  breakfast."  The  idea  is  that  the  reward  en- 
courages the  development  of  a  sense  of  anticipation  and  of  the  ability  to 
generalize  about  time—characteristics  that  the  typical  schizophrenic  cliild 
does  not  have. 

"The  climate  is  child -centered  in  that  it  responds  sensitively  to  Hie 
child's  needs,"  Dr.  Goldfarb  reports.  "However,  it  is  adult-directed  in  thai 
it  is  characterized  by  active,  assertive,  adult  direction,  and  it  is  the  adult 
who  determines  and  delineates  the  environment  for  the  child.  The  atmos- 
phere thus  contrasts  sharply  with  a  permissive,  nondirective  environment. 
The  child  is  reminded  at  all  times  of  adult  expectations.  The  whole  ideal* 
to  correct  the  child's  abnormal  ideas  and  behavior  whenever  they  are 
expressed,  at  the  moment  they  are  expressed -all  day  long." 

As  an  example,  a  staff  member  found  Mary,  mentioned  earlier  as  being 
abnormally  anxious  about  her  body,  in  panic  because  she  had  bumped  liei 
arm  and  had  seen  a  slight  red  bruise  appear.  "I'll  die,"  Mary  cried 
repeatedly.  "I'm  going  to  bleed  to  death."  Reassuring  words  did  no  good, 
so  the  adult  gravely  took  her  arm,  bent  the  elbow  gently  several  limes, 

306 


examined  each  finger  carefully,  and  finally  informed  her  he  was  sine  he 
could  make  the  arm  better  in  a  few  minutes.  He  asked  Mary  to  watch  the 
red  mark  get  lighter  as  he  patted  the  arm.  She  watched  eagerly  and  her 
tears  subsided,  but  she  continued  to  ask  if  she  would  die.  The  staff 
member  informed  her  authoritatively  that  there  was  absolutely  no  pos- 
sibility of  her  dying  from  the  bruise.  As  proof,  he  showed  her  that  the 
redness  was  already  disappearing.  The  girl  complained  that  the  arm  still 
hurt.  In  that  case,  he  told  her,  there  was  one  more  thing  he  could  do.  He 
ceremoniously  proceeded  to  put  cold  water  on  the  injured  area.  Patting 
the  arm  dry,  he  asked,  "Doesn't  that  feel  better  now?"  Mary  looked  at  her 
arm  and  then  up  at  him  and  said,  "Yes,"  and  walked  quietly  to  her 
classroom. 

The  recurrent  puzzlement,  often  accompanied  by  panic,  shown  by  the 
schizophrenic  child  reflects  an  inability  to  see  a  relation  between  the 
experience  to  which  he  is  being  exposed  and  some  earlier  experience.  It 
also  reflects,  Dr,  Goldfarb  continues,  a  desire  to  see  such  a  relation— to 
find  a  pattern  and  familiarity  in  his  experiences.  So  an  expression  of 
puzzlement  in  the  adult's  cue  to  offer  explanations  and  to  help  the  child 
evaluate  a  particular  event.  "Very  much  like  a  parent  of  an  infant,"  the 
investigator  says,  "the  staff  member  uses  warmth,  affection,  comforting, 
and  physical  contact  to  reinforce  the  learnings  involved  and  to  improve 
the  range  of  pleasure  in  and  tolerance  for  informational  input." 

Along  with  corrective  socialization,  the  Center  uses  drugs  in  some  cases 
and  individual  psychotherapy  in  virtually  all.  And  it  works  with  the 
parents  in  an  effort  to  clear  up  their  own  puzzlements  and  anxieties,  and 
thus  enable  them  to  develop  a  healthful  home  environment. 

Three-fourths  of  the  children,  as  reported  earlier,  do  return  home. 
Detailed  information  about  their  progress  awaits  an  analysis,  which  is 
under  way,  of  observations  over  the  last  10  years.  However,  the  Center 
already  knows  that  most  of  the  children  need  special  help,  such  as  a 
special  school  01  a  special  class  to  maintain  them  in  the  community.  These 
findings,  then,  are  indications  at  once  of  progress,  of  problems,  and  of 
opportunities. 


Day  Treatment  Versus  Residential:  Comparison  of  Results 

The  day  treatment  center  was  opened  in  1959  in  coopeiation  with  the 
New  York  State  Department  of  Mental  Hygiene.  It  is  the  equivalent  of  a 
day  hospital.  It  cares  for  its  children  from  9  to  4:30,  5  days  a  week  and  9 
months  a  year.  During  that  time  it  provides  the  same  kind  of  detailed, 
on-the-spot  intervention  as  provided  in  the  residential  treatment  program. 
For  approximately  20  children,  the  day  center  has  3  child  psychiatrists,  a 
psychiatric  supervisor,  and  3  teachers.  Caseworkers  serve  the  families  as 
they  do  those  of  the  other  children.  Extensive  psychological  services  are 
provided . 

The  two  types  of  treatment  have  been  compared  by  Dr.  Goldfarb  and 
his  associates  on  the  basis  of  changes  in  two  groups  of  children  over  a 

307 


3-year  period-one  group  living  at  the  center,  the  other  going  there  days. 
Each  group  had  13  children  matched  as  to  sex,  age  at  admission  to  treat- 
ment, neurological  diagnosis,  and  intellectual  functioning  at  admission. 

Three  of  the  children  in  each  group  were  so  retarded  in  intellectual 
functioning  that  no  full  IQ  score  could  be  obtained.  Two  of  these  in  each 
group  were  classified  as  organic,  while  the  classification  of  the  third 
remained  in  doubt.  On  the  basis  of  psychiatric  appraisals  and  the  results  of 
tests,  these  six  nonscorable  children  showed  no  improvement  over  the 
3-year  program,  regardless  of  the  treatment  program. 

Of  the  othei  20  children,  6  in  each  treatment  group  were  classified  as 
organic  and  4  as  nonorganic.  The  following  findings  pertain  to  these  20: 
•  Most  of  the  children  classified  as  organic  showed  improvement,  both 
as  rated  by  psychiatrists  and  as  indicated  by  tests  of  reading  achieve- 
ment and  IQ.  The  extent  of  the  improvement  was  apparently  not 
influenced  by  the  type  of  tieatment  program.  In  reading,  the  organic 
children  in  the  day  center  went  from  an  average  standard  score  of  % 
to  one  of  129;  those  in  the  residential  program,  from  74  to  132.  The 
day  children  showed  an  average  IQ  of  62  upon  admission,  reached  an 
average  of  74  at  the  end  of  the  second  year,  and  then  dropped  to  an 
average  of  65  at  the  end  of  the  third  year.  Similarly,  the  oiganlc 
children  in  residence  went  from  an  average  IQ  of  66  to  one  of  80  at 
the  end  of  the  second  year,  and  then  dropped  to  72.  (These  drops, 
after  the  initial  rises,  are  unexplained.) 

«   None  of  the  nonorganic  children  in  the  day  program  showed  any 
improvement  as  rated  by  psychiatrists.  Three  of  the  four  in  the  re- 
sidential program  did  show  such  improvement.  The  children  it)  each 
treatment  group  improved  in  leading,  but  those  in  the  residential 
group  far  more— from  89  to  182,  as  compared  with  from  98  to  137 
for  the  day  children.  As  for  the  IQ  scores,  the  children  in  each  group 
started  with  an  average  in  the  upper  80's  and  gained  a  few  points  in 
the  first  2  years.  In  the  third  year  the  average  IQ  of  the  residential 
children  shot  up  to  103,  while  that  of  the  day  children  remained  92. 
In  sum,  the  bulk  of  the  schizophrenic  children  who  had  brain  damage, 
but  were  sufficiently  intact  to  be  scorable  on  an  IQ  test,  improved  to 
about  the  same  degree  whether  in  the  day  or  the  residential  program.  The 
nonorganic  children,  on  the  other  hand,  did  substantially  better  in  the 
residential  program. 

These  differences  can  be  explained,  the  investigators  suggest,  by  dif- 
ferences in  the  families.  The  family  of  the  nonorganic  child  has  been  a 
more  fundamentally  disturbing  influence,  from  which  it  benefits  him  to 
get  away  completely.  But  the  organic  child  seems  not  to  need  complete 
separation ;  he  can  progress  if  he  is  given  an  educational  and  environmental 
experience  with  which  he  can  cope,  and  if  his  family  is  given  professional 
assistance  in  contending  with  his  deviancy  and  meeting  his  needs. 

The  most  efficient  treatment  of  schizophrenic  children,  these  findings 
suggest,  calls  for  two  types  of  facilities:  One  offering  a  day  program  for 
children  of  the  organic  type,  and  one  offering  long-term  impatient  treat- 
ment for  those  children  whose  deviant  manifestations  are  explained  lo  a 

308 


large  extent  by  psychosocial  factors.  In  making  these  suggestions,  the 
investigators  point  out  that  the  samples  in  this  study,  which  is  being 
repeated,  were  small  and  that  all  the  children  came  from  intact  families. 
Other  confirmatory  studies  are  urged. 

Research  Grant.  MH  5753 
Date  of  Interview.  Dec.  28,  1965 

References' 

Goldfarb,  W.  Childhood  Schizophrenia.  Cambridge:  Harvard  University  Press,  1961. 

Published  for  the  Commonwealth  Fund. 
Goldfarb,  W.  The  mutual  impact  of  mother  and  child  in  childhood  schizophrenia. 

Amer.J.  Orthopsychiat,,  1961,31,4. 

Goldfarb,  W.  Families  of  schizophrenic  children.  Ment.  Retard.,  1962,  39. 
Goldfarb,  W.  Self-awareness  in  schizophrenic  children.  Arch.  Gen,  Psychiat.,  1963,  8. 
Goldfarb,  W.  Childhood  schizophrenia.//!/.  Psychiat.  Clinics,  1964,  1,  4. 
Goldfarb,  W.  Corrective  socialization:  A  rationale  for  the  treatment  of  schizophrenic 

children.  Canacl,  Psychiat,  Association  J  ,  1965,  10,  6. 
Goldfarb,  W.,  &  Goldfarb,  N.  Evaluation  of  behavioral  changes  of  schizophrenic 

children  in  residential  treatment.  Amei.  J  Psycho  ther,,  1965,  19,2. 
Goldfarb,  W.,  Goldfarb,  N.,  &  Pollack,  Ruth  C.  Treatment  of  childhood  schizophrenia. 

A  3-year  comparison  of  day  and  residential  treatment.  Arch.  Gen.  Psychiat.,  1966, 

14. 
Goldfarb,  W.,  Levy,  D.M.,  &  Meyeis,  D.I.  The  verbal  encounter  between  the  schizo- 

phienic  child  and  his  mother.  Presented  at  the  20th  Anniversary  Meeting  of  the 

Psychoanalytic  Clinic  for  Training  and  Research  at  Columbia  University,  1965.  In 

press,  1966. 
Goldfarb,  W.,  &  Mintz,  I.  Schizophrenic  child's  reactions  to  time  and  space.  Arch,  Gen. 

Psychiat.,  1961,5. 
Goldfarb,  W.,  &  Pollack,  Ruth  C.  The  childhood  schizophrenic's  response  to  schooling 

in  a  residential  treatment  center.  Evaluation  of  psychiatric  treatment.  New  York. 

Grime  and  Stratton,  1964. 
Meyers,  D.I.,  &  Goldfarb,  W.   Studies  of  perplexity  in  mothers  of  schizophrenic 

children.  Amei.  J.  Orthopsychiat.,  1961,31,3. 

Meyers,  D.I.  &  Goldfarb,  W.  Psychiatric  appraisals  of  parents  and  siblings  of  schizo- 
phrenic children.  Amei:  J.  Psychiat.,  1962,  1 18,  10. 
Taft,  L.T.,  &  Goldfarb,  W.  Prenatal  and  perinatal  factors  in  childhood  schizophrenia. 

Developm.  Med.  Child  Neural,  1964,  6. 


309 


Investigators. 

William  Pollin,  M,D. 
James  R.  Stabenau,  M.D. 
Loren  IVIosher,  M.D. 
JoeTupin,  M.D. 
Axel  Hotter,  M.D. 
Martin  Allen,  M.D. 
Barbara  Scupi,  M.S.W. 
NIMH 


Prepared  by: 

Maya  Pines 

When  one  identical  twin  become  schizophrenic,  but  the  other  does  not, 
what  in  their  environment  caused  the  difference?  And  when  both  twins 
become  schizophrenic,  what  in  their  biochemical  make-up  predisposed 
them  to  it? 

By  asking  provactive  questions  like  these,  Dr.  William  Pollin  and  liis 
associates  at  the  Section  on  Twin  and  Sibling  Studies,  Adult  Psychiatry 
Branch  of  NIMH,  hope  to  uncover  major  pieces  of  the  jigsaw  puzzle  that  is 
schizophrenia  -  a  disease  which  accounts  for  roughly  half  of  all  hos- 
pitalized mental  patients  in  the  world.  They  are  not  primarily  interested  in 
twins,  but  in  schizophrenia,  and  for  the  past  six  years  they  have  been 
conducting  a  series  of  strictly  controlled  studies  that  combine  the 
techniques  of  psychiatry,  genetics,  and  biochemistry.  These  studies  focus 
on:  (1)  the  specific  family  patterns  that  contribute  to  triggering  schizo- 
phrenia in  one  twin,  but  not  in  another,  and  (2)  the  underlying  bio- 
chemical abnormalities  that  are  shared  by  both  twins.  Thus,  they  point 
the  way  to  two  fruitful  lines  of  attack  on  this  widespread  disease. 

On  the  biochemical  side,  they  are  currently  evaluating  the  possible 
significance  of  their  most  recent  finding  on  1 1  pairs  of  identical  twins;  the 
high  rate  of  catecholamines  excreted  in  the  urine  of  both  the  schizo- 
phrenic and  the  healthy  twin.  The  catecholamines  have  two  important 
functions:  Some  of  them,  particularly  norepinephrine  (noradrenaline) 
carry  messages  to  different  parts  of  the  brain;  and  all  of  them  play  an 


essential  role  in  the  body's  efforts  to  cope  with  stress.  For  this  reason, 
previous  reports  of  high  catecholamines  in  psychiatric  conditions  had  been 
attributed  to  the  effects  of  the  patients'  anxiety.  However,  stress  usually 
produces  other  signs  as  well  -  for  instance,  an  elevation  of  adrenal  steroids. 
Yet  the  healthy  twins  showed  no  parallel  rise  in  their  adienal  steroids, 
indicating  that  the  high  rate  of  catecholamine  excietion  was  not  solely  a 
response  to  stress.  And,  indeed,  other  analyses  in  this  Lab  determined  that 
the  amount  of  catecholamines  in  both  twins  was  under  genetic  control. 
This  strongly  suggested  that  overactivity  of  the  catecholamine  system  did 
not  merely  reflect  psychosis,  but  might  help  to  produce  it. 

The  Sibling  Studies:  Schizophrenia  vs.  Delinquency 

As  Dr.  Pollin  points  out,  it  is  very  rare  in  behavioral  research  to  have 
really  well-controlled  comparative  studies.  This  represents  a  serious 
problem,  particularly  since  there  are  so  few  well-anchored,  reliable 
measures  of  personality  or  of  psychopathology.  Psychiatrists  can  clinically 
get  the  impression  that  a  certain  kind  of  life  course  is  very  relevant  to  a 
psychosis,  but  trying  to  nail  down  the  mechanisms  involved  is  extremely 
difficult. 

He  began,  therefore,  by  studying  siblings,  on  the  theory  that  children  of 
the  same  parents  would  provide  a  better  basis  for  comparison.  He  wanted 
to  compare  the  circumstances  that  led  a  child  in  each  of  three  sets  of 
families  to  become  either  schizophrenic,  delinquent,  or  exceptionally  well 
adjusted,  while  another  child  in  the  same  family  was  just  normal.  "In 
some  ways,  delinquency  and  schizophrenia  are  polar  oppositcs,"  he 
explains.  "Schizophrenia  is  a  disease  in  which  inner  representations  of  the 
external  world  become  completely  distorted,  resulting  in  delusions  and 
hallucinations.  Delinquency  is  a  pathology  in  which  one's  relations  to  the 
outer  world  are  disordered.  It  is  the  difference  between  acting  in  and 
acting  out."  However,  nonspecific  reactions  of  guilt  and  shame  are  present 
in  the  families  of  both. 

The  research  scheme  devised  by  Dr.  Pollin  together  with  Drs,  James  R. 
Stabenau  and  Joe  Tupin  fitted  in  with  the  trend  towards  focusing  not 
solely  on  the  schizophrenic  patient,  but  on  the  patient  and  his  family  as  a 
unit.  It  involved  three  groups  of  five  selected  families.  The  ten  youngsters 
in  each  group  were  carefully  matched  for  age  (between  14  and  18),  sex, 
sibling  order,  and  social  class.  All  were  Protestant.  All  were  being  raised  by 
their  biological  parents.  Both  parents  and  both  siblings  in  each  family 
participated  in  all  phases  of  the  research.  The  five  schizophrenics  ("S" 
ndexes)  had  been  diagnosed  as  schizophrenic  by  at  least  two  psychiatrists 
ind  had  suffered  from  hallucinations,  marked  paranoid  delusions,  or 
narked  regression  for  6  months  to  9  years.  The  five  delinquents  (*'D" 
ndexes)  were  referred  by  legal  agencies;  they  had  been  charged  with  car 
heft,  promiscuity,  or  running  away.  The  exceptionally  well-adjusted 
/oungsters  ("N"  indexes)  had  been  nominated  by  the  staffs  of  a  local 
unior  college  and  high  school  for  their  outstanding  academic  work  and/or 
personality  adjustment  and  peer  relationships.  With  the  exception  of  the  S 

311 


indexes,  all  were  seen  during  outpatient  visits,  and  all  participants, 
including  the  15  normal  contiol  siblings,  were  interviewed  and  tested  ill 
length. 

Fiom  the  outset,  very  different  patterns  emerged  among  the  three  sets 
of  families  -  patterns  which  appeared  to  antedate  the  symptoms  of 
psychological  disturbance  in  S  and  D  families.  Among  the  D  families,  for 
instance,  all  relationships  were  unstable.  There  were  no  fixed  roles  or 
responsibilities  for  anyone,  and  it  was  uncertain  who  would  be  the  father, 
who  the  mother,  who  would  dominate  or  be  the  leader  at  any  time.  By 
contrast,  the  S  parents  seemed  locked  in  a  rigid  relationship  to  each  other 
and  to  their  children.  It  was  a  kind  of  stalemate.  On  the  other  hand,  the  N 
parents  seemed  flexible,  empathic,  and  able  to  complement  one  another. 
On  tests  of  communication  and  clear  thinking,  such  as  the  Object  Sorting 
Test  and  the  Revealed  Differences  Test,  both  the  S  and  D  families  did 
poorly  compared  to  the  N  families. 

The  family  histories  of  the  three  groups  revealed  even  more  striking 
differences.  In  nearly  every  S  or  D  family  (but  not  in  the  N  families),  a 
major  crisis,  spontaneously  characterized  by  the  family  as  "the  worst 
time,"  had  occurred  when  the  index  child  was  between  6  months  and  3 
yeais  of  age,  usually  reaching  its  peak  at  about  the  time  he  was  1 8  months 
old.  "One  of  the  important  consequences  of  this  differing  incidence  of 
family  ciisis  appeared  to  be  its  potential  for  serving  as  the  ougin  of  a 
negative  identity  in  a  child  through  identification  with  a  depressed,  guilty, 
and  anxious  mother,"  wrote  Drs.  Stabenau  and  Pollin.  "In  addition,  the 
disruptiveness  of  the  family  life  at  that  time  further  served  to  reduce  Hie 
sense  of  internal  security  for  the  index  child." 

During  these  earliest  years,  too,  the  parents  in  the  S  group  often  viewed 
the  child  who  later  became  schizophrenic  as  either  physically  or  psy- 
chologically "damaged."  (Few  of  the  non-schizophrenic  children  were 
considered  defective  in  this  fashion.)  And  nearly  all  the  children  who  later 
became  either  S  or  D  had  younger  siblings  born  within  their  first  two 
years,  while  few  of  the  controls  did. 

But  why  did  schizophrenic  symptoms  develop  in  one  group,  and  delin- 
quent symptoms  in  another,  when  both  suffered  from  early  family  crises? 
The  researchers  point  to  differing  family  life  styles  and,  in  the  case  of 
delinquents,  to  an  additional  crisis:  the  sudden  collapse  of  the  role  of  the 
father,  just  before  the  child  began  to  act  out.  Depending  on  the  timing  of 
this  change,  one  child  within  the  family  would  become  delinquent  while 
*i *i.^r  wouid  not_  jn  one  cas6)  for  example,  a  father  who  had  kept  his 


from  a  parent,  close  sibling  or  grandparent.  Often  the  mother  scrutinized 
the  index  child's  activities  with  phobic  concern  about  where  he  was  and 
with  whom  he  played,  fearing  harm  at  play  or  his  being  killed  in  the 
streets.  This  resulted  in  depriving  him  of  the  freedom  to  explore  the  world 
around  him  -  a  freedom  granted  within  broad  limits  to  N  childien,  and 
given  almost  recklessly  to  D  children.  Typically,  the  index  child's  life  style 
was,  and  had  been,  extremely  constricted. 

In  addition  to  differences  such  as  these  in  the  patterns  of  family 
relationships,  existing  evidence  of  a  genetic  factor  in  schizophrenia 
suggests  that  certain  organic  factots  must  have  been  present  in  the  S 
families,  though  not  in  the  others. 


An  Intensive  Study  of  Identical  Twins  Discordant  for  Schizophrenia 

As  long  as  the  subjects  were  genetically  different,  these  findings  had 
limited  genera  Usability  because  inherited  tiaits  might  account  for  most  of 
the  differences  between  them.  In  1962,  Drs.  Pollin  and  Stabenau  deter- 
mined to  repeat  their  study  -  this  time,  with  identical  twins.  One-egg 
twins,  they  believed,  would  represent  "the  optimal  controlled  sample,"  in 
which  not  only  genetic  factors  but  also  social,  ethnic,  chronological  and 
psychological  variables  were  matched  to  a  degree  not  attainable  in  other 
ways. 

Twins  have  been  studied  with  great  interest  ever  since  antiquity.  The 
ancients  who  believed  in  astrology  were  intrigued  by  the  fact  that  twins 
born  at  almost  the  same  time  could  have  contrasting  life  histories.  To 
explain  this  divergence,  they  decided  that  major  changes  in  the  configura- 
tion of  the  heavenly  bodies  must  have  occurred  precisely  in  the  interval 
between  the  births  of  Twin  1  and  Twin  2.  This  meant  that  if  they  could 
locate  identical  twins  with  differing  life  histories  and  find  out  the  moment 
of  their  birth,  they  could  pinpoint  the  limits  of  major  astronomical 
periods.  Therefore,  they  actively  sought  out  and  studied  such  twins.  But  it 
was  only  in  the  19th  century,  with  Francis  Gallon,  that  the  classical  twin 
method  came  into  use  as  a  method  of  measuring  the  relative  contributions 
of  heredity  and  environment  to  a  specific  illness  or  other  condition. 

Since  monozygotic  (MZ)  or  identical  twins  are  genetically  alike,  but 
dizygotic  (DZ)  or  fraternal  twins  are  genetically  no  more  similar  than  any 
other  siblings,  the  degree  of  concordance  between  twins  of  both  types  is 
meaningful.  If  40  out  of  50  pairs  of  MZ  twins  are  concordant  for  an 
illness,  but  only  10  out  of  50  pairs  of  DZ  twins  are  so  concordant,  the 
difference  between  these  two  figures  is  a  measure  of  the  genetic  contri- 
bution to  the  illness.  However,  this  assumes  that  the  environment  of  MZ 
twins  could  be  considered  to  have  been  constant  -  a  major  flaw  in  the 
method,  according  to  Dr.  Pollin. 

"We  set  out  differently,"  he  explains.  "We  did  not  try  to  evaluate  the 
relative  importance  of  the  genetic  factor,  but  to  ignore  it,  in  the  sense  that 
it  was  controlled  for  both.  We  were  thus  in  an  optimal  position  to  study 
the  non-genetic  factors  involved."  It  took  very  extensive  recruiting 

313 


through  hospitals,  university  psychiatric  departments,  and  other  sources 
to  find  enough  pans  of  identical  twins  in  which  one  was  healthy  but  the 
other  was  adjudged  clearly  schizophrenic  by  five  psychiatrists,  and  in 
which  both  parents  were  willing  to  come  to  Bethesda  to  be  studied  for 
two  or  three  weeks  at  the  NIH  Clinical  Center,  together  with  both  twins. 
But  finally  16  such  families  were  brought  to  the  Center  and  evaluated, 
together  with  9  control  families. 

Having  at  least  these  key  members  of  the  family  present  is  "almost 
essential  if  one  hopes  to  obtain  a  meaningful  historical  leconstruction 
concerning  the  differential  experiences  of  the  two  twins,"  declare  the 
researchers.  "Much  of  the  more  important  data  concerns  the  mother's 
piegnancy  and  the  birth  and  first  years  of  life  of  the  twins,  facts  known 
only  to  the  parents.  In  the  absence  of  prolonged  psychoanalytic  relation- 
ship with  the  parents,  such  material  appears  to  be  most  accessible  via  a 
family-focused  evaluation  situation  in  which  four  family  members  are 
constantly  supplementing,  stimulating,  and  correcting  the  material  that 
each  recollects  and  presents."  Besides,  they  could  be  given  complete 
psychiatric,  psychological,  and  biological  work-ups,  including  tests  of 
blood,  urine,  and  chromosomes  and  some  25  different  psychological  tests. 
The  twins'  zygesity  was  determined  by  investigating  28  blood-group 
factors,  as  well  as  fingerprints  and  10  anatomical  features.  Over  30  differ- 
ent collaborating  investigators  participated  in  the  study. 

The  schizophrenic  twin  was  admitted  to  an  inpatient  ward.  If  actively 
psychotic  at  the  time  of  the  study,  in  most  cases  he  received  lengthy 
treatment  at  the  Clinical  Center  at  no  cost  to  the  family.  The  other 
members  of  his  family  stayed  in  a  physically  identical  ward  on  another 
floor.  The  nonschizophrenic  twin  was  often  interested  in  being  evaluated 
because  he  wanted  to  find  out  his  chances  of  remaining  healthy. 

Ever  since  Dr.  Franz  Kallmann  described  his  genetic  theory  of  schizo- 
phrenia in  the  1940's  and  stated  that  the  concordance  rate  for  schizo- 
phrenia was  approximately  80  percent  -  that  if  one  identical  twin  became 
schizophrenic,  his  co-twin  was  likely  to  develop  the  same  symptoms  in  80 
percent  of  the  cases  -  these  figures  have  stuck  in  the  public  mind, 
producing  great  fear  among  affected  families.  However  in  1963,  after  a 
careful  study  of  1 6  pairs  of  MZ  twins  in  which  one  was  schizophrenic,  tlie 
Finnish  investigator  Pekka  Tienari  reported  that  none  of  the  co-twins  was 
affected.  Previous  studies  had  traced  twins  through  mental  hospitals; 
Tienari  stalled  out  with  parish  records  of  twin  births  and  followed  them 
up,  This  may  have  accounted  for  some  of  the  difference  in  results,  since 
the  overburdened  families  of  two  schizophrenic  patients  would  be  more 
likely  to  have  them  committed  to  a  mental  hospital  than  the  families  of 
one.  A  larger  study  by  Einar  Kringlen  in  Norway  followed  up  all  the  twins 
born  in  a  given  area  during  a  given  decade  and  found  the  concordance  rate 
for  MZ  twins  to  be  38  percent,  substantially  lower  than  any  reported 
before  1960. 

Like  the  Scandinavian  work,  Dr.  Pollin's  current  analysis  suggests  that 
instead  of  being  concordant  for  schizophrenia  in  80  percent  of  the  cases, 
MZ  twins  are  actually  discordant  for  the  illness  in  approximately  75 

314 


percent  of  cases.  Even  at  the  time  of  his  earliest  twin  studies,  he  was  able 
to  reassure  the  healthy  twins  about  their  prospects,  and  while  all  members 
of  the  family  found  the  study  stressful,  in  most  cases  the  healthy  twins 
left  the  Center  with  their  fears  lessened. 

These  figures  focused  interest,  once  again,  on  the  psychodynamic 
factois  involved  in  schizophrenia.  Tienari  had  noted  that  the  twins  who 
were  psychologically  more  submissive  were  the  ones  to  develop  schizo- 
phrenia in  all  but  one  of  his  16  cases.  Kallmann  and  others  had  made 
similar  observations.  The  NIMH  study  of  twins  essentially  confirms  this 
insight,  and  also  explains  how  and  why  the  environment  of  the  stricken 
twin  differed  from  that  of  the  healthier  twin,  almost  from  the  time  of 
conception. 

In  a  detailed  report  on  the  first  five  families  studied  in  1964,  Drs. 
Pollin,  Stabenau,  and  Tupin  described  "a  consistent  pattern  of  historical 
events  and  related  familial  attitudes"  which  distinguished  the  index  from 
his  healthy  co-twin: 

1 )  The  twin  who  later  became  schizophrenic  weighed  less  at  birth. 

2)  He  was  perceived  by  his  parents,  particularly  by  his  mother,  as 
vulnerable,  and  his  survival  was  thought  to  be  imperiled. 

3)  He  was  the  focus  of  more  worry,  involvement,  and  attention  than  his 
co-twin. 

4)  He  developed  somewhat  more  slowly. 

5)  He  tended  to  perform  less  successfully,  and  to  be  perceived  as  the 
less  competent  and  weaker  of  the  twins. 

6)  He  tended  to  be  the  more  docile  and  more  compliant  of  the  two,  was 
less  independent,  and  had  difficulty  in  achieving  any  degree  of  autonomy 
and  separateness. 

7 )  These  relative  differences  tended  to  be  persistent  and  unchanging. 
This  pattern  resulted  in  part  from  constitutional  diffciences,  and  in  part 

from  "a  rigidly  'imprinted'  role  expectation,  initiated  at  birth,  determined 
by  the  constitutional  differences,  and  subsequently  reinforced  by  minor 
differences  in  development,"  the  authors  report.  They  see  it  as  largely  the 
result  of  a  self-fulfilling  prophecy. 

In  all  five  instances,  the  mother  had  a  strong  conscious  fear  of  death 
concerning  the  index  twin  and/or  herself,  immediately  after  his  birth.  In 
two  of  these  cases  there  was  a  brief  period  of  legitimate  concern  about  the 
index  twin's  survival,  as  he  remained  in  the  hospital  for  several  days  after 
his  co-twin  was  taken  home.  In  another,  however,  the  mother  would  not 
accept  the  pediatrician's  assurance  that  both  twins  were  fine  and  that  the 
smaller  one,  because  of  her  size,  would  necessarily  feed  more  slowly  and 
take  less  formula  -  she  had  a  phobic  anxiety  that  the  smaller  twin  would 
die  if  she  did  not  feed  her  as  frequently  and  as  much  as  the  other. 

These  worries  led  the  parents  to  concentrate  their  efforts  on  the  smaller 
twin.  In  each  of  the  five  families,  the  mother  felt  that  the  smaller  twin 
"needed  her  more."  When  she  fed  both  twins,  she  fed  the  smaller  one 
first.  If  only  one  could  be  breast-fed,  it  would  be  the  smaller  one.  In  two 
instances,  the  mothers  reporting  pinching  and  slapping  the  smaller  twin 
and  using  cold  water  on  him  to  keep  him  awake  so  he  would  eat  more. 

315 


Once  established,  this  pattern  persisted  as  the  twins  grew  up.  "The 
smaller  twin  would  receive  extra  praise  for  things  taken  for  granted  in  the 
larger  one,"  the  researchers  report.  "He  would  receive  additional  help  with 
schoolwork,  and  would  be  less  expected  to  dress  himself  or  take  respon- 
sibility for  personal  needs  or  household  chores."  The  bigger  twins  were 
always  a  bit  more  successful  in  school  and  social  life.  They  played  the 
leader  role  and  made  all  the  decisions.  They  were  also  less  docile  and 
"good."  Only  the  healthy  twins  had  episodes  of  active  rebellion  or  acting 
out.  One,  for  example,  engaged  in  violent  controversies  with  his  father, 
in  which  the  index  twin  never  participated.  The  index  twins  never  showed 
non-compliance;  if  they  expressed  it  at  all,  it  was  passively,  in  such  a  way 
that  the  parents  could  rationalize  it  as  just  another  symptom  of  their 
disability. 

Since  the  twins  appeared  identical  in  so  many  ways,  "the  needs  of  all 
concerned  to  find  distinguishing  identities  for  them  led  to  a  sharpening 
and  highlighting  of  such  initial  differences,11  note  Drs.  Pollin,  Stabenau, 
and  Tupin.  They  reflect  that  if  the  healthy  twins  had  not  been  present  for 
nearly  constant  comparison,  the  parents  might  not  have  perceived  the 
index  twins  as  so  weak  or  vulnerable,  but  might  have  accepted  them  as 
normal.  Their  anxiety  and  intense  involvement  with  the  smaller  twin  often 
led  the  parents  to  ambivalent  feelings  towards  him.  They  tended  to 
project  the  negative  side  of  their  self-image  onto  this  smaller  twin  moie 
often  than  on  the  larger  one. 

This  general  pattern  tended  to  hold  true  as  the  study  progressed.  In  a 
report  on  1 1  families  with  MZ  twins  discordant  for  schizophrenia  in  1 965, 
the  authors  declared  that  each  of  the  1 1  index  twins  was  the  smaller  one 
at  birth.  The  difference  in  birth  weights  ranged  from  1/2  ounce  to  I 
pound  1 2  ounces.  This  lower  weight  "appears  to  reflect  some  lower  level 
of  anatomic  development  and/or  differentiation,  and  consequently,  a 
lesser  physiological  competence  and  stability  present  at  birth,"  they 
reported.  The  index  twins  also  had  a  marked  preponderance  of  such 
problems  as  cyanosis,  infantile  colic,  feeding  difficulties,  burns,  multiple 
fractures,  and  severe  illnesses  which  knocked  them  flat  for  months  at  a 
time.  Their  parents  recalled  that  these  smaller  twins  had  "worried  more," 
cried  more,  been  more  "fussy,"  and  seemed  more  sensitive.  Almost 
without  exception,  and  from  earliest  childhood,  the  index  twins  were 
described  as  more  dependent,  more  submissive,  more  fearful,  more 
compliant,  and  more  constricted  than  their  heavier  co-twins. 

To  help  refresh  their  memories,  the  parents  were  asked  to  bring  in 
whatever  pictures  or  home  movies  they  had  available.  ("It  seems  all 
families  have  large  numbers  of  pictures  in  shoe  boxes,"  says  Dr.  Pollin. 
"They  may  never  have  put  the  pictures  into  albums,  but  they  have 
them.")  Nevertheless,  there  remained  the  problem  of  retrospective 
distortion.  To  alleviate  it,  a  social  worker  lived  with  the  families  in  their 
homes  a  couple  of  days,  then  went  to  see  as  many  relatives,  doctors, 
teachers,  and  friends  as  possible,  accumulating  up  to  25  interviews  per 
family  to  get  a  more  objective  view  of  the  past.  Often  she  found  that 
many  of  those  who  knew  the  family  recalled  the  bigger  twin  as  an  easier 

316 


child  to  deal  with.  Almost  from  birth,  the  smaller  twin  actually  had  quite 
a  different  life  course  -  as  he  must  have  had  a  different,  less  favorable 
intrauterine  experience. 

It  is  quite  normal  for  one  twin  to  be  smaller  than  the  other  at  birth. 
Thus,  many  parents  fall  into  the  pattern  described  above  because  of 
realistic  concern  for  a  vulnerable  child.  However,  they  change  this  pattern 
after  the  child  gains  in  strength  and  maturity.  It  is  only  in  certain  cases, 
when  paients  are  too  troubled  or  rigid  to  modify  their  picture  of  the 
weaker  twin,  that  the  pattern  becomes  potentially  dangerous.  Apparently 
something  about  the  weaker  twin's  situation  resonates  with  a  particular 
problem  in  these  parents'  lives,  relighting  an  intense,  unresolved  conflict. 
No  matter  how  much  the  pediatrician  may  reassuie  them,  they  remain 
consumed  with  anxiety  about  the  child.  On  the  other  hand,  they  find 
dealing  with  the  stionger  twin  an  easy  task  -  a  distinct  relief,  In  this  way 
the  twins  experience  different  models  of  parental  behavior,  and  have 
increasingly  divergent  experiences. 

In  a  later  report  (1967),  Drs.  Pollin  and  Stabenau  discuss  two  additional 
sets  of  identical  twins  in  which,  surprisingly,  the  twin  who  became  schizo- 
phrenic did  not  weigh  less  at  birth.  They  find  it  very  significant  that, 
despite  a  favorable  start,  these  children  suffered  from  specific  stresses 
which  reversed  their  relative  position  and,  in  effect,  made  them  weaker 
than  the  lighter  twin.  In  one  of  these  cases,  the  index  twin  had  turned 
blue  from  cyanosis  because  of  poisoning  from  a  defective  heater  next  to 
her  bed  and  had  been  taken  to  a  hospital,  near  death,  while  her  co-twin, 
who  slept  in  another  part  of  the  room,  suffered  only  minor  effects.  In  the 
other,  the  twin  who  was  heavier  at  birth  nearly  died  from  a  severe  case  of 
Rocky  Mountain  spotted  fever  at  age  3  and  1/2,  after  which  there  was 
great  concern  regarding  his  health  and  survival. 

Thus,  whether  it  began  before  biith  -  through  diffeiences  in  fetal 
positioning  and  consequent  crowding,  differences  in  fetal  circulation,  or 
other  factois  that  produced  relative  physiological  incompetence  -  or 
through  accident  in  early  childhood,  life  presented  very  different 
experiences  to  these  "identical"  twins.  Between  the  ages  of  2  and  6,  the 
stronger  twin  was  usually  the  more  verbal  and  the  more  independent. 
From  6  to  adolescence,  the  weaker  twin  often  became  increasingly 
dependent  on  his  healthier  co-twin  -  who  simultaneously  began  to  turn  to 
others  for  friendship.  In  late  adolescence  and  early  adulthood,  the  healthy 
twin  moved  further  towards  individuality  and  heterosexual  relationships. 
This  accentuated  a  sense  of  loneliness  and  despair  in  the  weaker  twin. 

"Often,  disorganization,  withdrawal,  and  schizophrenic  symptoma- 
tology develop  in  the  lighter  birth  weight  twin  at  just  this  time,  i.e.,  when 
the  heavier,  more  differentiated  twin  is  making  a  sudden  spurt  in  the 
development  of  an  individual  identity  and  the  establishment  of  hetero- 
sexual and  genital  level  of  personality  organization,"  write  Stabenau  and 
Pollin.  To  the  index  twin,  the  world  had  always  appeared  more 
threatening  -  and  its  stresses  mounted  all  around  him  as  he  grew  up. 

317 


The  Veterans'  Study:  15,000  Pairs  of  Twins  Discordant  for  Schizophrenia 

In  1967,  the  Medical  Follow-Up  Agency  of  the  National  Academy  of 
Sciences-National  Research  Council  made  available  to  researchers  its 
national  registry  of  all  the  pairs  of  white  male  twins  born  between  1917 
and  1927  who  served  in  the  Armed  Forces  during  World  War  II  or  t!ie 
Korean  War.  It  was  a  list  drawn  from  the  54,000  pairs  of  twins  born 
during  that  decade.  It  excluded  23,000  pairs  of  twins  who  did  not  serve  i\\ 
the  Armed  Forces  at  all,  as  well  as  15,000  pairs  in  which  only  one  twin 
served.  This  left  15,930  pairs  of  twins  healthy  enough  for  both  to  liave 
passed  the  physical  and  mental  tests  leading  to  induction  into  the  service. 

The  records  of  these  men  yielded  a  wealth  of  information  that  could  be 
fed  into  computer  tape:  Induction  physical  examinations;  inpatient  and 
outpatient  hospital  and  clinic  diagnoses  during  their  period  of  services;  VA 
hospital  diagnoses  after  they  left  the  service;  and  diagnoses  based  on 
responses  to  a  questionnaire.  They  were  between  38  and  48  years  old  in 
1965,  after  a  follow-up  period  averaging  1 8  to  20  years. 

Among  these  15,930  pairs  of  twins,  Drs.  Pollin,  Martin  G.  Allen,  Axel 
Hoffer,  and  their  associates  found  338  pairs  in  which  one  or  both  twins 
had  been  diagnosed  as  schizophrenic  at  some  time  after  entry  to  active 
duty.  In  some  cases  their  zygosity  was  unknown,  but  226  pairs  could  be 
identified  as  identical  (MZ)  or  fraternal  (DZ)  twins.  Among  the  80  MZ 
pairs,  11,  or  1 3,8  percent,  were  concordant  for  schizophrenia.  Among  the 
146  DZ  pairs,  only  6,  or  4.1  percent,  were  concordant  for  the  disease.  The 
concordance  rate  for  schizophrenia  was  thus  3.3  times  greater  among  MZ 
pairs  than  among  DZ  pairs.  By  contrast,  the  concordance  rate  for  neurosis 
was  almost  the  same  in  both  kinds  of  twins  - 10.7  percent  for  MZ  pairs  as 
compared  to  7.1  percent  for  DZ  pairs. 

A  re-analysis  of  1 8  earlier  major  twin  studies  showed  a  similar  ratio  in 
all  but  one  instance,  suggesting  "the  presence  of  a  genetic  factor  in  the 
pathogenesis  of  schizophrenia,  and  its  relative  absence  in  psych  on  euro  sis," 
report  the  researchers.  "However,  since  approximately  85  percent  of 
affected  MZ  pairs  in  the  NRC  sample  are  discordant  for  schizophrenia,  the 
role  of  the  suggested  genetic  factor  appears  to  be  a  limited  one." 

This  large  sample  of  twins  also  allowed  Drs.  Allen  and  Pollin >  to  their 
own  surprise,  to  cast  doubt  on  one  of  the  most  widely  accepted  notions  of 
the  psychodynamic  determinants  of  schizophrenia  in  analytic  theory:  the 
"diffuse  ego  boundary"  hypothesis.  This  holds  that  schizophrenia  is  due 
to  a  "confusion  of  identity"  resulting  from  weak  ego  boundaries,  and  thai 
such  problems  are  greater  in  twins,  especially  MZ  twins.  Therefore,  MZ 
twins  could  be  expected  to  have  a  higher  incidence  of  schizophrenia  than 
T>7  twins,  and  all  twins  would  be  expected  to  have  a  higher  incidence  ofit 


hypothesis,  they  do  not  actually  refute  it,  either,  since  possibly  other 
factors  unique  to  twin  personality  development  might  offset  whatever  ego 
boundary  defects  exist. 

The  Transmission  of  Schizophrenia 

"It  is  very  easy,  in  work  with  family  dynamics,  to  say,  Aha,  this  is  the 
difference  in  the  behavior  of  the  mother  and  father  that  leads  to  schizo- 
phrenia -  these  are  the  schizophrenia-producing  characteristics  of  parents. 
However,  that  is  not  the  way  we  think  of  it  right  now,"  warns  Dr.  Pollin. 
Parental  factors  are  only  one  among  a  variety  of  ctiologic  factors  that  may 
play  a  role  in  lowering  resistance  to  the  disease  or  triggering  it,  he 
explains. 

For  years  there  has  been  an  ideological  struggle  between  psychiatrists 
who  believe  that  schizophrenia  results  from  some  genetic,  biochemical 
impairment,  and  those  who  believe  it  comes  from  faulty  child-rearing.  Dr. 
Pollin  sees  a  great  need  to  integrate  both  points  of  view. 

So  far  there  is  no  definitive  evidence  that  parental  behavior  is  the 
predominant  factor,  he  points  out.  Whether  or  not  it  is  a  necessary  factor 
cannot  be  determined  until  enough  prospective  studies  have  been 
completed.  Much  of  the  disturbance  seen  in  the  parents  of  schizophrenics 
may  be  secondary  to  the  disease,  rather  than  a  cause  of  it.  All  that  can  be 
said  with  assurance  so  far  is  that  certain  family  patterns  seem  to  ac- 
company schizophrenia.  However,  these  patterns  cannot  be  blamed  for 
the  disease.  It  may  be  that  there  was  some  defect  in  the  child  from  the 
beginning  -  a  defect  we  do  not  yet  know  how  to  recognize  or  define. 

Nor  are  the  patterns  of  family  interaction  that  Dr.  Pollin  described  the 
only  possible  ones  in  the  development  of  schizophrenia.  They  may  repre- 
sent only  one  of  several  different  patterns  leading  to  the  same  effect.  By 
choosing  to  work  only  with  families  in  which  both  parents  and  both  twins 
were  willing  and  able  to  come  to  the  NIH  Clinical  Center  for  an  extended 
period  of  time,  he  narrowly  limited  his  sample.  As  he  makes  clear,  the 
incidence  of  schizophrenia  tends  to  be  high  among  broken  families  and 
those  with  a  great  deal  of  strife  and  schizms  -  the  very  kind  he  had 
eliminated  from  his  study.  "We  dealt  with  families  where  over- 
involvement  with  a  child  and  over-protectiveness  were  more  likely  to  be  a 
factor,"  he  notes,  "If  we  had  not  required  the  families  to  participate,  we 
might  have  found  other  patterns,  centered  on  more  overt  types  of 
rejection." 

Illnesses  can  be  passed  from  one  person  to  another  in  many  different 
ways.  They  can  be  transmitted  by  a  microbe,  as  in  bacterial  infection; 
genetically,  as  in  phenylketonuria;  socially,  in  the  sense  that  poor,  socially 
backward  slum  families  live  in  conditions  that  are  likely  to  produce  a  high 
incidence  of  TB;  or  by  various  combinations  of  these  factors. 

To  understand  the  transmission  of  schizophrenia,  it  helps  to  look  at 
other  illnesses,  Dr.  Pollin  believes.  He  particularly  likes  the  model  de- 
veloped by  two  English  researchers  to  explain  the  incidence  of  a  very 
different  illness;  congenital  dislocation  of  the  hip. 

319 


Though  much  simpler  and  more  concrete,  congenital  dislocation  of  tli 
hip  clearly  has  genetic  as  well  as  environmental  factors,  as  does  schizc 
phrenia.  It  runs  in  families,  with  identical  twins  concordant  for  it  in  41 
percent  of  cases.  Yet  the  majoiity  of  twin  pairs  are  discordant  for  tli- 
disease.  Drs.  Cedric  0.  Carter  and  John  A.  Wilkinson  of  the  Mcdiea 
Genetics  Unit,  British  Research  Council,  were  able  to  tease  apait  tin 
vanous  factors  involved. 

They  found,  first  of  all,  that  certain  aspects  of  the  anatomy  of  the  liij 
joint  weie  controlled  by  heredity.  If  the  hip  joint  is  visualized  as  a  kind  oi 
modified  ball  and  socket,  the  shape  of  the  socket  -  its  depth,  and  thcsiz* 
of  its  roof  -  is  the  key  to  a  good  fit.  Obviously,  the  shallower  the  sockcl 
and  the  shorter  its  roof,  the  easier  for  the  head  of  the  thigh  bone  (the  ball 
in  it)  to  pop  out.  Yet  this  shape  was  determined  by  a  genetic  factor.  Thus, 
a  genetic  factor  produced  an  anatomical  predisposition  for  the  illness. 

Next,  investigating  the  fact  the  dislocation  of  the  hip  was  eight  or  nine 
times  more  frequent  among  girls  than  among  boys,  they  found  ;i  gencwl- 
ized  laxness  in  all  the  joints  of  female  infants,  which  they  traced  to  a  (low 
of  hormones  from  the  endocrine  glands  of  girl  babies  just  before  birtli. 
These  hormones  temporarily  loosened  the  infants'  connective  tissues. 

The  condition  was  also  much  more  common  among  children  born  by 
breech  presentation,  which  bent  their  legs  in  a  position  that  favored  the 
thigh  bone's  popping  out,  The  custom  of  swaddling  had  similar  effects, 
mis-directing  pressure  on  the  baby's  legs;  this  accounted  for  the  high  rate 
of  dislocation  of  the  hip  among  certain  American  Indian  tribes. 

Among  the  Chinese  in  Hong  Kong,  on  the  other  hand,  dislocation  of 
the  hip  was  rare.  The  researchers  traced  this  to  the  custom  of  carrying 
infants  in  a  back  sling  which,  far  from  loosening  the  hip  joint,  tended  to 
push  the  ball  back  into  the  socket. 

When  twins  were  discordant  for  the  illness,  it  often  turned  out  that  one 
twin  had  been  carried  in  a  back  sling,  while  the  other  had  not;  one  Iwd 
been  swaddled,  and  the  other  had  not;  or  else,  being  of  different  sexes, 
they  had  had  different  levels  of  hormones  at  birth.  With  MZ  twins,  Ilir 
most  common  difference  was  their  manner  of  birth:  because  of  Ihc 
intrauterine  mechanics  involved,  one  would  be  born  by  breech  picscnla* 
tion,  while  the  other  was  not. 

Though  the  differences  between  schizophrenia  and  congenital  dis- 
location of  the  hip  are  obvious,  Dr.  Pollin  believes  one  can  draw  some 
cautious  but  useful  analogies  between  the  two  illnesses.  In  bolh  cases, 
there  are  many  different  pathways  which  lead  to  the  same  abnormal 
structure,  The  development  of  the  human  ego,  like  that  of  the  hip  joint, 
can  be  impeded  by  genetic  factors,  intrauterine  mechanisms,  environment 
in  early  infancy,  social  customs,  accidents,  or  various  combinations 
thereof, 

It  should  also  be  noted  that  "schizophrenia"  is  a  shorthand  word  for 
various  forms  of  mental  illness,  he  points  out.  When  Bugcn  Blculer 
introduced  it  in  1 9 1 1 ,  he  used  the  plural:  "The  Group  of  Schizophrenia*." 
It  is  still  generally  believed  that  schizophrenia  includes  several  cliffcren!, 
though  overlapping,  disease  entities.  Eventually,  when  the  total  picture 

320 


becomes  clearer,  several  different  patterns  of  family  interaction  and  bio- 
chemical characteristics  may  be  recognized  as  leading  to  different  forms  of 
schizophrenia. 

The  Possibility  of  Prevention 

The  fact  that  the  majority  of  twins  with  the  genotype  for  schizophrenia 
do  not  become  schizophrenic  shows  that  some  kind  of  intervention  is 
possible:  Most  life  experiences  do  not  lead  to  the  development  of  schizo- 
phrenia. "However,  so  far  we  don't  even  know  what  specific  biochemical 
changes  take  place  at  the  time  of  the  psychotic  break,"  points  out  Dr. 
Pollin.  "Nor  do  we  understand  why  LSD,  in  microamounts,  can  cause  a 
break  with  reality  and  a  florid  psychotic  reaction.  We  are  still  fumbling  in 
the  dark  -  as  though  we  were  trying  to  treat  heart  disease  without 
understanding  the  basic  mechanism  of  the  heart." 

Within  ten  years,  our  knowledge  of  the  factors  that  contribute  to  a  high 
risk  of  schizophrenia  should  be  at  about  the  same  level  as  our  present 
knowledge  of  the  various  factors  that  contribute  to  a  coronary,  Dr.  Pollin 
speculates.  He  hopes  that  it  will  then  be  possible  to  state  with  some 
certainty  which  combinations  of  factors  represent  a  risk  high  enough  to 
warrant  the  use  of  preventive  drugs,  or  which  factors  in  early  childhood 
need  to  be  modified. 

"If  you  understand  the  specific  steps  that  lead  to  a  disease,  you  can 
approach  its  therapy  and  prevention  from  a  rational  point  of  view,'*  he 
says.  "But  our  treatment  of  schizophrenia  has  been  quite  the  opposite  —  all 
entirely  empirical  or  accidental.  People  have  stumbled  on  methods  of 
treatment.  The  tranquilizers  were  found  during  a  search  for  a  new  type  of 
anti-histamine.  If  one  depends  on  such  accidents,  the  odds  against  finding 
the  most  rational  treatment  and  prevention  measures  are  very  bad.  We 
must  define  more  precisely  the  specific  factors  that  contribute  to  a  high 
risk." 

Dr.  Pollin  cites  the  work  of  Dr.  Sarnoff  Mednick,  Professor  of  Psychol- 
ogy New  York  School  for  Social  Research,  New  York,  New  York,  with 
children  of  schizophrenic  mothers  as  an  example  of  the  kind  of  studies 
which  offers  the  best  hope  of  finding  clues  to  prevention.  The  children  of 
schizophrenic  mothers  are  a  high-risk  population.  Dr.  Mednick  expects 
that,  out  of  his  first  series  of  200  children,  at  least  30  will  become  schizo- 
phrenic within  15  or  20  years.  However,  instead  of  having  to  depend  on 
their  relatives'  memories,  he  will  have  a  complete  record  of  the  children's 
pre-illness  characteristics  and  of  the  conditions  under  which  they  were 
raised.  This  will  allow  him  to  see  what  differentiates  the  children  who 
become  sick  from  those  who  stay  healthy.  So  far,  Dr.  Mednick Js  results  fit 
in  very  well  with  Dr.  Pollin's  for  he  has  found  a  clear  correlation  between 
the  development  of  schizophrenia  and  the  kind  of  pregnancy  and  birth 
difficulties  which  might  have  caused  damage  to  tlie  patient's  central 
nervous  system  either  before  or  during  birth.  Given  a  genetic  predis- 
position to  schizophrenia,  such  insults  to  the  central  nervous  system  might 
well  be  the  factor  that  triggers  the  disease. 

321 


For  the  past  three  years,  Drs.  Pollin,  M.  Allen,  and  D.  Cohen  have  * 
carrying  out  a  prospective  study  of  their  own  to  better  uiuleislimil 
development  of  personality  characteristics  and  family  lelationships  win 
when  accompanied  by  other  factors  -  might  play  a  role  in  pioilm 
schizophrenia.  However,  unlike  Dr.  Mednick,  they  do  not  anticipate 
cases  of  schizophrenia  among  their  subjects.  They  are  simply  studying 
origins  of  certain  patterns  in  the  early  life  of  twins.  Having  eunliu. 
obstetricians  in  the  Washington  area  about  women  who  expected  nuilt 
births,  they  asked  the  parents'  cooperation  and  then  tlid  [iron, 
interviews  in  the  parents'  homes.  One  of  the  psychiatrists  in  tin-  M?CI 
was  present  at  the  time  of  each  multiple  delivery.  If  the  twins  v. 
identical,  the  researchers  kept  careful  jecords  of  exactly  how  I  lie  lu 
differed  at  birth,  how  the  birth  process  itself  varied,  the  ncumlufi 
findings  on  each,  and  their  behavior  in  the  nursery.  They  followed  up 
twins  regularly,  first  every  few  weeks,  then  every  few  months  at  fun 
and  from  time  to  time  the  twins  were  brought  to  the  NIH  Clinical  ('en 
for  extensive  tests  and  films. 

"We  wanted  to  define  exactly  when  the  differences  between  iileuli 
twins  became  consistent.  We  found  that  one  of  them  became  mi 
dominant,  more  skillful  with  objects,  more  comfortable  with  slr.iHfi 
less  fearful,  at  a  very  early  age,"  declares  Dr.  Pollin.  "Our  fit  si  juimp 
twins,  10  pairs,  is  now  between  2  and  3  years  old,  and  we  aie  oitltMi 
our  second  sample  of  10.  We  hope  that  by  the  time  they  are  5  VOM 
we  will  be  able  to  pull  together  some  useful  generalizations," 

Biochemical  Abnormalities  that  May  Predispose  to  Schizophrenia 

Recent  studies  by  Dr.  Pollin  have  singled  out  abnormalities  in  I 
catecholamine  system  as  a  genetically  determined  factor  whidi  in 
contribute  to  the  development  of  schizophrenia  -  and  perhaps  loiHli 
psychoses.  In  Leningrad,  in  the  summer  of  1970,  he  ivpnilol  I 
preliminary  findings  on  19  pairs  of  identical  twins,  of  whom  II  vu 
discordant  for  schizophrenia,  4  were  concordant  (both  selil/oplin-Jii 
and  4  were  normal  controls.  All  were  inpatients  at  the  NIH  C'link'ul  fun 
in  Bethesda,  where  their  diets  were  similar  and  samples  for  nearly  iill  |'j 
of  twins  were  obtained  on  the  same  day. 

Each  identical  twin  excreted  nearly  the  same  amount  of  cnlecliohimr 
as  his  co-twin.  The  following  catecholamines  were  analysed:  l)<H\i?nr 
(the  precursor  from  which  norepinephrine  and  epinephrinc  lire  nwimf; 
tured);  norepinephrine;  epinephrine;  normetanephrine;  mclanephririiv 
VMA.  Theintraclass  correlation  coefficients- which  measure 
which  intrapair  similarity  in  identical  twins  is  greater  than  the  Mi 
between  persons  who  are  not  genetically  related  -  were  high  and  sU 
ly  significant  at  values  that  ranged  from  <.05  to  <.001 ,  indicating  ll>.it 
levels  of  these  substances  were   under  a  significant  degree  of  ik'ii<-'i 
control, 

The  similarity  remained  even  when  one  identical  twin  WJK  ^h"'- 
phrenic  and  the  other  was  not.  For  the  1 1  discordant  MZ  pairs  nCb*)11 

322 


the  intraclass  correlation  coefficients  were  +.79  for  norepinephrine,  +.77 
for  dopamine,  +.62  for  normetanephrine,  and  +.80  for  metanephrine. 
"The  degree  of  genetic  control  present  is  not  submerged  or  obscured  by 
the  presence  or  absence  of  schizophrenia,"  leported  Dr.  Pollin.  "It  is 
especially  high  for  norepinephrine  and  dopamine," 

This  was  particularly  interesting  by  comparison  with  the  MZ  twins' 
discordant  production  of  adrenal  steroids.  The  catecholamines  play  two 
important  roles  in  the  body:  (1)  Some  of  them  (norepinephreine  and 
dopamine,  those  most  clearly  under  genetic  control)  act  as  neurotrans- 
rnitters  in  the  central  nervous  system,  e.g.,  norepinephrine  in  the  hy- 
pothalamus,  the  area  where  emotional  activities  aie  integrated  with  higher 
abstract  activities,  and  (2)  All  of  them  are  known  to  rise  in  response  to 
stress.  However,  they  usually  rise  together  with  other  responses  by  the 
adrenal  glands.  Yet  in  this  case,  the  healthy  MZ  twins  did  not  show  any 
abnormally  high  level  of  17-OH  steroids;  only  their  schizophrenic  co-twins 
did.  Thus,  the  high  levels  of  catecholamines  in  both  twins  could  not  be 
attributed  entirely  to  the  stress  of  the  immediate  situation.  In  Dr.  Pollin 's 
hypothesis,  they  may  represent  one  of  the  underlying  factois  that 
predispose  to  schizophrenia. 

Researchers  have  long  thought  that  it  would  make  sense  if  some  of  the 
steps  leading  to  serious  psychopathology  were  located  in  the  system  that 
responds  to  stress.  For  if  one  is  stressed  and  becomes  anxious,  beyond  a 
certain  point  this  anxiety  can  itself  reduce,  rather  than  increase,  one's 
ability  to  cope  with  stress.  As  one's  ability  to  cope  with  stress  decreases, 
the  external  threat  appears  greater,  leading  to  a  greater  response,  and 
eventually  to  a  pathological  spiral. 

A  man  who  sees  a  lion  come  charging  at  him  responds  with  a  sudden 
rush  of  epinephrine  (also  called  adrenaline)  and  other  catecholamines. 
This  brings  about  many  physiological  changes.  His  blood  is  massively 
shunted  from  other  parts  of  the  body  into  the  muscular  system,  equipping 
him  to  run  twice  as  fast  as  he  otherwise  could.  His  energy  is  mobilized  for 
either  fight  or  flight.  Useful  and  adaptive  as  this  response  may  be  to  a 
caveman  who  often  faces  dangers  requiring  physical  prowess,  it  becomes 
self-defeating  in  our  society,  where  the  threats  are  more  complex,  subtle, 
and  chronic.  Studies  in  Dr.  Pollin's  Lab  have  shown  that  high  levels  of 
circulating  epinephrine  lead  to  a  constriction  of  the  perceptual  field  -they 
make  one  see  less.  Again,  the  ability  to  focus  on  a  wild  animal  to  the 
exclusion  of  everything  else  might  prove  quite  useful  in  a  jungle,  but  in  a 
complex  situation  it  would  be  better  to  increase  one's  ability  to  make 
subtle  abstract  distinctions  —  the  kind  of  ability  which  is  impaired  by  high 
levels  of  epinephrine.  These  high  levels  of  epinephrine  also  diminish  the 
precision  of  man's  reaction  to  stress,  leading  him  to  over-react  in  in- 
creasingly non-productive  ways.  In  this  fashion  they  can  produce  the  stage 
of  exhaustion  described  by  Hans  Seyle,  in  which  all  ability  to  adapt  to 
stress  is  lost. 

323 


It  is  therefore  quite  possible  that  the  hyper-secretion  of  catechol 
impedes  the  development  of  the  ability  to  cope  with  stress,  and  tin 
to  schizophrenia.  This  might  be  one  of  the  mechanisms  involved,  t< 
with  the  non-genetic  factors  discussed  above.  In  cases  of  discc 
among  MZ  twins,  perhaps  one  of  the  twins  develops  techniques  t 
with  the  hypothetically  higher  level  of  catecholamines,  while  tht 
does  not;  then,  as  a  result  of  his  larger  number  of  successes,  he  devc 
normal  fashion.  Meanwhile  his  less  successful  co-twin  becomes  inci 
ly  dependent,  submissive,  and  constricted,  and  in  the  absence  of 
kinds  of  intervention  from  family  or  friends,  the  direction  of  his  d< 
ment  may  become  irreversible. 

Many  interesting  and  important  leads  tend  to  implicate  the  cat* 
mines  in  mental  illness,  Dr.  Pollin  points  out.  For  instance,  mosl 
that  are  able  to  cause  an  artificial  psychosis  are  methylated  derivat 
amines.  Thus,  mescaline  is  a  methylated  derivative  of  dop 
Methionine,  one  of  the  essential  amino  acids,  is  believed  to  be  es 
specifically  because  it  supplies  the  methyle  group,  a  very  common 
which  plays  an  important  role  in  body  chemistry.  And  it  was  sh< 
this  Lab  nine  yeais  ago  that  when  large  quantities  of  methioni 
-administered  to  schizophrenic  patients,  some  of  these  patients  will  s 
severe  exacerabation  of  their  psychosis.  (This  is  one  of  th 
biochemical  findings  about  schizophrenia  on  which  there  is  general 
ment.)  This  leads  to  the  hypothesis  that  some  abnormal,  mctti 
metabolites  of  the  catecholamines  are  formed  in  the  body  of  i 
persons  because  of  a  genetically  determined  fault;  that  one  of 
metabolites  is  chemically  similar  to  a  psychotomimettc  drug;  and  til 
metabolite  -  perhaps  a  methylated  derivative  of  epinephrine  —  can 
psychotic  symptoms.  However,  this  remains  to  be  proved. 

Another  lead  comes  from  Parkinsonism.  One  of  the  side  effects 
effective  anti-psychotic  drugs  known  to  date  is  that  they  produce  (r 
similar  to  Parkinsonism.  It  was  originally  believed  that  these  tremo 
rigidity  were  an  allergic  response  to  the  drugs.  But  it  has  now  been  s 
that  if  anti-psychotic  drugs  are  given  in  high  enough  dosage,  sor 
percent  of  all  patients  will  develop  Parkinsonism.  The  biochemical  b; 
Parkinsonism  has  recently  been  established  -  it  is  a  deficien 
dopamine,  which  among  other  things  serves  an  essential  neurotransi 
role  in  the  basal  ganglia,  the  part  of  the  brain  associated  with  ] 
activity.  So  Parkinsonism  implies  a  lower  level  of  dopamine.  A  lowci 
of  dopamine  implies  a  lower  level  of  all  the  catecholamines,  since  i 
body  dopamine  is  the  substance  from  which  the  other  catecholamin 
derived.  The  drug  L-Dopa  treats  Parkinsonism  by  increasing  the  le 
dopamine.  Interestingly,  it  has  had  ill  effects  on  some  schizopl 
patients.  In  the  past  two  years  it  has  been  used  on  a  fair  number  of 
patients,  not  to  treat  their  illness,  but  to  treat  the  severe  Parkinsonisi 
symptoms  developed  by  some  2  or  3  percent  of  schizophrenics  as  a 
of  liigh  dosages  of  thorazine.  In  a  significant  number  of  cases,  L- 

324 


exacerbated  the  psychosis.  This,  once  again,  seems  to  indicate  that  in- 
creased levels  of  dopamine  and  other  catecholamines  help  to  produce  the 
symptoms  of  schizophrenia. 

If  indeed  an  inherited  biochemical  fault  involving  high  levels  of  catechol- 
amines predisposes  certain  persons  to  schizophrenia,  it  is  a  fault  that 
might  become  manifest  only  if  the  system  were  working  at  high  pressure  — 
which  brings  one  right  back  to  the  intrauterine  experiences  and  family 
factors  with  which  Dr.  Pollin  began. 

"There  is  something  unfortunately  seductive  about  biochemical  work," 
notes  Dr.  Pollin,  an  analyst  who  does  not  find  the  analytic  and 
biochemical  approaches  conflicting,  but  complementary.  "At  least  it  can 
be  quantified !  We  have  fascinating  life  histories  about  our  twins,  each  one 
a  novella,  but  presenting  it  in  a  hard,  piecise  way  is  infinitely  more 
difficult." 

He  points  out  that  whether  the  research  is  approached  from  the  angle 
of  biochemistry  or  that  of  family  patterns,  two  issues  remain  unclear: 

1 )  Can  his  findings  be  replicated  with  a  larger  sample?  and 

2)  Which  of  his  findings  aie  specific  to  schizophrenia,  rather  than  to  all 
psychoses? 

His  next  study  will  try  to  get  at  these  issues.  From  the  large  pool  of 
twins  made  available  by  the  NAS-NRC,  he  hopes  to  select  four  matched 
groups  of  twins:  one  group  of  twins  who  are  discordant  for  schizophrenia, 
another  who  are  discordant  for  depression,  a  third  who  are  discordant  for 
severe  neurosis,  and  another  who  are  normal.  Each  group  will  consist  of 
about  20  pairs  of  twins.  Through  extensive  biochemical  and  psychological 
comparisons  of  these  four  groups,  Dr.  Pollin  hopes  to  determine  which 
factors  are  specific  to  schizophrenia,  and  which  arc  common  to  all  severe 
psychopathology. 

Research  Grant:  Intramural 

Date  of  interviews:  December  1970,  January  1971 

References: 

Allen,  M.,  and  Pollin,  W.  Schizophrenia  m  twins  and  the  diffuse  ego  boundary  hy- 
pothesis, American  Journal  of  Psychiatry,  111  (4): 43 7-443,  1970. 

Hoffer,  A.  and  Pollin,  W.  Schizophrenia  in  the  NAS-NRC  panel  of  15,909  veteran  twin 
pairs,  Archives  of  General  Psychiatry,  23:469-477,  1970. 

Mosher,  L.,  Poltin,  W.,  and  Stabenau,  I.  R.  Identical  twins  discordant  for  schizo- 
phrenia: Neurological  findings.  In  press,  A rchives  ofGeneiaiPsy chiatry. 

Mosher,  L.,  Pollin,  W.,  and  Stabenau,  J.  Families  with  identical  twins  discordant  for 
schizophrenia:  Some  relationships  between  identification,  thinking  styles,  psycho- 
pathology  and  dominance-submissiveness.  British  Journal  of  Psychiatry t  1 18:29-42, 
1971. 

Pollin,  W.  The  unique  contribution  of  twin  studies  to  the  elucidation  of  non-genetic 
factors  in  personality  development  and  psychopathogenesis.  Acta  Genetic  Medicae 
et  Gemellogie,  19  (l-2):299-303,  1970. 

Pollin,  W.  and  Stabenau,  J.  Biological,  psychological  and  historical  differences  in  a 
series  of  monozygotic  twins  discordant  for  schizophrenia.  In  Kety,  S.  S.,  and 
Rosenthal,  D.  (eds.).  The  Transmission  of  Schizophrenia  London,  Pergamon  Press 
Ltd.,  317-332,  1968. 

Pollin,  \V.  Possible  genetic  factor  related  to  psychosis.  For  presentation  May  7,  1971, 
Annual  Meeting  of  the  American  Psychiatric  Association,  Washington,  D.C. 

325 


W.  A  new  approach  to  the  use  of  twin  study  data  in  studies  of  the  pathogen. 

of  schizophrenia  and  neurosis.  In  Kaplan,  A.  (ed  )    Genetic  Factors  in  Scht 

phrenw.  To  be  published  by  Chas.  C.  Thomas  Press 
Pollfii,  WM  Allen,  M.  G.t  Hoffer,  A.,  Stabenau,  J.  R.,  and  Hrubec,  Z.  PsychopaJhdc 

in  1  5,909  pairs  of  veteran  twins.  Evidence  for  genetic  factor  in  the  pathogenous 

schizophrenia  and  its  relevative  absence  m  psych  on  eurosis.  American  Journal 

Psychiatry.  126,  #5:597-61 1,  1969. 
Stabenau,  J.  R.  and  Pollin,  W,  Early  characteristics  of  monozygotic  twins  discordant  I 

schizophrenia.  Archives  of  General  Psychiatry.,  17:723-734,  1967. 
Stabenau,  J.  R    and  Pollin,  W.  Maturity  at  birth  and  adult  protein  bound  lodir 

Nature,  215.996-997,  1967. 
Stabenau,  L  R.  and  Pollin,  W,  Comparative  life  history  differences  of  families  i 

schizophrenics,    delinquents,  and    "normals."  American  Journal  of  Psyclttatr) 

124:11,  1968. 
Stabenau,  J.  R.,  Pollin,  W.,  and  Mosher,  L.  A  study  of  monozygotic  twins  disco «l a r 

for    schizophrenia'    Some  biologic   variables.  Archives   of  General   PsycJitatr) 

20:145-158,1969. 
Stabenau,  J.  R.,  Tupin,  J,,  Wener,  M.,  and  Pollin,  W.  A  comparative  study  offiimilit 

of  schizophrenics,  delinquents,  and  normals.  Psychiatry,  28(l):4S-59,  196S. 


326 


We  shall  do  so  much  in  the  years  to  come, 
But  what  have  we  done  today? 
We  shall  give  our  gold  in  a  princely  sum. 
But  what  did  we  give  today? 


-Nixon  Waterman 


Investigator: 

Joseph  D.  Teicher,  M.D. 

University  of  Southern  California 

School  of  Medicine 

Los  Angeles,  Calif. 

Prepared  by 

Gay  Luce 


Using  interviews  and  psychological  tests,  a  research  team  has  compared 
50  adolescents  after  an  attempted  suicide,  with  unsuicidal  peers  of  the 
same  age,  sex,  and  background.  Although  economic  privation,  broken 
homes,  and  disciplinary  problems  were  found  in  the  control  group-the 
sequence  and  timing  of  events  occurred  at  a  different  phase  in  the  devel- 
opment of  the  child.  The  profile  of  the  suicidal  adolescent  includes  long- 
standing problems  with  family,  a  stage  of  escalation  during  adolescence, 
and  a  final  stage  of  alienation-a  chain  reaction  that  dissolves  the  adoles- 
cent's closest  personal  bonds.  Given  detailed  biographical  knowledge  of  an 
adolescent,  this  study  indicates  that  it  should  be  possible  to  pick  out  the 
youth  in  danger,  for  adolescent  suicide  is  not  irrational  but  over- 
determined  by  sequences  of  life  events  occurring  in  critical  periods. 


Background 

Adolescent  suicide  is  horrifying,  unthinkable,  and  a  little  unreal  to  most 
adults,  for  we  tend  to  be  complacent  about  the  troubles  of  the  young.  To 
the  modern  adults,  Romeo  and  Juliet  may  seem  only  a  story.  Yet  many 
adolescents  cling  to  one  another  in  similar  love,  with  the  desperation  of  a 
last  hope  in  a  lonely  world.  A  modern  Juliet  is  likely  to  be  a  frightened 
and  pregnant  little  girl;  the  boy  is  likely  to  be  rejected,  and  both  may  feel 
totally  alone. 

Literary  descriptions  of  childhood  suicide  seem  bizarre,  yet  they  re- 
semble modern  case  histories.  In  Thomas  Hardy's  Jude  the  Obscure,  the 
restless  wanderings  and  misery  of  unmarried  parents  overcome  an  un- 
wanted oldest  boy.  When  he  hears  that  yet  another  unwanted  baby  is 
coming,  he  kills  himself  and  the  other  children.  It  is  not  that  such  events 
don't  happen,  but  we  are  reluctant  to  believe  them. 

In  1965,  Jacobziner  estimated  that  there  were  60,000  attempted  sui- 
cides among  young  people  under  age  20  in  the  United  States  each  year. 

329 


Adolescence  can  be  a  particularly  lonely  and  difficult  period,  a  time  of 
biological  upheaval  and  social  change.  A  person  is  expected  to  emerge 
from  the  safety  and  dependency  of  childhood  into  responsible  maturity. 
Even  healthy  and  happy  adolescents  become  moody  and  oscillate  between 
passions  and  depressions  in  a  manner  that  the  older  people  around  them 
rarely  understand.  Most  adolescents  have  fantasies  about  killing  them- 
selves in  moments  of  rage  and  frustration  or  when  they  feel  totally  iso- 
lated from  their  families  and  friends.  This  is  not  surprising.  Who  has  not 
imagined,  with  some  glee,  the  remorse  his  parents  would  feel  if  he  killed 
himself?  Between  such  imaginings  and  the  act  lies  the  world  of  pathological 
events  that  Doctor  Teicher  and  his  associates  have  begun  to  define. 

Statistics  portray  great  misery  among  a  large  population  of  adolescents. 
Suicide  ranks  as  the  fourth  most  frequent  cause  of  death  for  young  people 
15—19  years  old.  Fortunately,  the  vast  number  of  attempted  suicides  in 
this  age  group  are  thwarted.  An  estimate  of  60,000  suicide  attempts  a  year 
may  seem  exaggerated,  but  hospital  admissions  offer  a  convincingly  sad 
picture.  In  1960,  for  instance,  at  New  York's  Bellevue  Hospital  attempted 
suicide  was  the  reason  for  admitting  10  percent  of  the  child  and  adoles- 
cent patients.  At  Kings  County  Hospital  in  Brooklyn,  13  out  of  every  100 
children  who  came  to  the  hospital  had  attempted  or  threatened  suicide. 
Each  month,  the  huge  Los  Angeles  County-U.S.C.  Medical  Center  admits 
about  seven  patients  between  14  and  18  who  have  attempted  to  kill 
themselves;  over  80  a  year. 


The  Attempted  Suicides 

There  has  been  a  general  tendency  to  dismiss  a  suicide  attempt  in  an 
adolescent  as  an  impulsive  act  stemming  from  a  temporary  crisis  or  depres- 
sion. Perhaps  it  is  soothing  to  believe  that  someone  so  young  with  "life 
ahead  of  him"  could  not  have  intended  to  kill  himself.  He  could  not  have 
considered  that  he  might  die.  On  the  contrary,  Doctor  Teicher  and  his 
associates  at  the  Medical  Center  of  the  University  of  Southern  California 
have  found  many  adolescents  who  attempted  to  take  their  lives  more  than 
once.  At  first  they  may  have  used  the  drastic  move  as  a  threat  to  draw 
attention  to  their  problems.  Instead,  it  generally  made  matters  worse. 
After  an  escalation  of  long-standing  problems  and  loss  of  any  meaningful 
relations,  many  concluded  that  death  was  really  the  only  solution  lo 
unsolvable,  unbearable,  and  chronic  problems. 

Beginning  with  Freud  around  1920,  many  keen  minds  in  the  develop- 
ment of  psychiatry  have  wrestled  with  the  problem  of  adolescent  suicide, 
but  inferences  drawn  from  a  few  cases  or  psychological  studies  did  not 
indicate  how  to  predict  a  suicide  from  outside  circumstances.  In  the  fall  of 
1964,  the  investigator  and  his  associates  began  to  study  the  life  situations 
of  adolescents  who  attempted  suicide,  comparing  them  with  control 
adolescents  matched  for  age,  race,  sex,  and  family  income -control 
adolescents  who  had  never  attempted  suicide,  Quite  a  few  interesting 
patterns  have  been  drawn  from  this  study  of  50  young  people  who 

330 


attempted  suicide.  All  were  between  14  and  18,  None  of  them  was 
mentally  retarded  or  obviously  pregnant.  All  had  been  brought  into  the 
Los  Angeles  County-U.S.C.  Medical  Center  sometime  between  September 
1964  and  May  1965  because  of  their  suicide  attempt. 

At  least  one  parent,  usually  the  mother,  was  studied  as  well.  For 
comparison  there  was  a  control  group  of  32  youngsters  and  their  parents. 
Three-quarters  of  the  attempted  suicides  were  girls.  On  the  average  the 
suicidal  adolescents  were  around  16  years  old.  They  were  white,  Mexican, 
Negro,  Protestant,  Catholic,  and  Jewish. 


Procedure:  Charts  of  Life  Events 

The  procedure  called  for  an  interview  with  the  adolescent  patient 
within  24  to  48  hours  after  the  suicide  attempt.  The  parent  or  parents 
were  also  interviewed.  Then,  the  suicidal  youngster's  therapy  sessions  in 
the  hospital  were  taped  and  transcribed  for  further  analysis. 

Two  biographies  were  elicited  from  structured  interviews.  There  was 
the  parent's  version  of  his  child's  history,  and  there  was  the  adolescent's 
version  of  his  own  life.  On  the  basis  of  the  case  histories,  a  life  history 
chart  was  constructed  for  each  suicide  attempter  and  his  matched  control. 
Tliis  was  done  by  constructing  a  chronology  (in  parallel)  on  a  vertical 
continuum  that  depicted  all  the  experiences  of  the  adolescent  from  birth 
until  the  suicide  attempt.  These  graphic  charts  show  residential  moves, 
school  changes,  the  beginnings  of  various  behavioral  problems,  separation, 
divorce,  or  remarriage  of  the  parents,  and  deaths  in  the  family.  The  charts 
were  put  in  a  sequence  that  displayed  how  the  events  tended  to  pile  up  at 
a  particular  point  in  the  adolescent's  life.  This  indicated  how  the  crises 
had  accumulated  during  the  adolescent's  life. 

What  events  distinguished  those  who  attempted  suicide  from  those  who 
did  not?  A  simple  comparison  of  events  in  the  lives  of  the  control  group 
and  the  suicide-attempters  might  not  show  that  there  was  a  very 
pronounced  difference.  The  investigators  discerned  a  distinct  process 
leading  to  progressively  deeper  unhappiness  and  pessimism.  The  suicide- 
attempters  went  through  a  sequence  that  led  to  progressive  isolation  from 
the  important  people  in  his  life.  The  control  adolescents  did  not.  The 
process  can  be  summarized  in  three  stages:  The  suicide-attempters  all  had 
a  long-standing  history  of  problems  from  chidlhood  into  adolescence. 
There  was  also  a  period  in  which  problems  seemed  to  escalate,  usually  at 
the  very  beginning  of  adolescence.  Moreover,  the  problems  mounted  in  a 
manner  that  seemed  to  exceed  those  of  peers  and  friends,  Finally,  came  a 
phase  characterized  by  a  "chain  reaction  dissolution  of  any  remaining 
meaningful  social  relationships."  This  isolation  occurred  in  the  days  and 
weeks  preceding  the  suicide  attempt. 

331 


Sequential  Analysis  of  Life  Events 

The  advantage  of  looking  at  things  sequentially  can  be  demonstrated  by 
comparing  the  two  groups.  For  instance,  the  life  histories  of  the  suicide- 
attempters  showed  that  72  percent  of  them  came  from  bioken  homes,  yet 
53  percent  of  the  control  group  also  came  from  broken  homes.  Former 
studies  of  suicide  have  emphasized  the  fact  that  there  were  more  broken 
homes  among  suicide  attempters  than  "control"  adolescents.  However 
none  of  these  studies  examined  the  broken  homes  of  comparison  groups, 
If  one  looked  only  at  the  incidence  of  broken  homes  and  severed  parental 
relations,  there  is  no  great  difference  between  suicidal  youths  and 
comparable  nonsuicidal  youths.  However,  by  looking  at  the  chronological 
biographies  of  these  two  groups,  the  grantees  have  seen  that  the  relevance 
of  a  broken  home  depends  upon  when  the  instability  occurred  in  Ihc 
child's  development. 


Critical  Phase 

Although  72  percent  of  the  suicide-attempters  and  53  percent  of  Ihc 
control  adolescents  came  from  broken  homes,  the  timing  of  divorce  and 
remarriage  was  different.  In  the  suicidal  group  58  percent  of  the  pnienls 
remarried,  but  only  one-fourth  of  the  control  parents  remarried.  More- 
over, these  control  parents  managed  to  remarry  very  early  in  the  child's 
life  and  remained  married.  The  parents  of  the  suicidal  adolescent  cilEter 
remarried  quite  a  bit  later  in  his  life,  or,  if  they  remarried  early,  they  were 
subsequently  divorced  and  remarried  several  times  again. 

The  chronological  mapping  of  biographies  shows  that  the  suicidal 
adolescents  ha'd  parents  who  were  divorced,  separated,  or  remarried  after 
the  onset  of  adolescence.  By  contrast,  the  control  families  experienced 
change  earlier,  if  at  all.  Instability  in  the  home  apparently  had  ;i  dif- 
ferential effect  depending  upon  the  age  of  the  child.  Both  groups  ex- 
perienced the  instability  of  a  broken  home,  but  the  nonsuicidal  adoles- 
cents had  a  stable  homelife  during  their  last  5  years,  while  the  suicidal 
youths  had  experienced  instability  then.  As  the  investigators  have 
written:1 

'This  is    particularly   significant,   not   only    because    divorce. 


person  to  depression  and  suicide  in  later  life.  Loss  of  a  love  object,  as  the 
grantee  has  remarked,  is  an  important  aspect  of  the  process.  But  loss  must 
be  viewed  as  a  part  of  the  process,  and  particular  attention  must  be  paid 
to  the  time  when  it  occurred.  Most  of  the  adolescents  began  their 
maelstrom  descent  toward  suicide  after  a  long  period  of  alienation  from 
parents.  One  14-year-old  who  had  tried  to  commit  suicide  twice  was  asked 
why.  She  replied,  "It's  my  mother."2 

Asked  what  her  mother  did,  she  answered,  "We  just  don't  get 
along.  We  haven't  for  3  years.  Before  that  we  were  like  sisters  and 
then  it  seems  like  since  she  divorced  my  stepfather  it  started  a  lot  of 
trouble." 

This  girl  enjoyed  being  in  the  hospital  and  did  not  want  to  return  home. 
It  is  particularly  poignant  that  she  wanted  to  be  committed  to  a  State 
mental  hospital  rather  than  return  home.  Many  of  the  young  suicide  at- 
tempters  described  their  alienation  from  parents  as  a  process  in  which 
either  the  mother  or  father  would  nag  them,  would  cut  them  off  from 
their  friends,  would  disapprove  of  their  favorite  friends,  and  thus  made  it 
difficult  for  them  to  have  relationships  outside  the  home,  at  the  same  time 
making  life  very  difficult  for  them  within  the  home.  This  was  their 
version. 


The  Broken  Romance 

Typically,  many  of  these  adolescents  had  fallen  in  love  and  formed  very 
possessive  and  exclusive  romantic  relationships.  This  actually  isolated 
them  even  more.  A  girl  and  boy  would  concentrate  so  intensely  on  one 
another  that  they  tended  to  cut  off  all  their  friends.  Then,  if  the  romance 
failed,  they  would  feel  hopeless,  lost  and  despairing. 

At  the  time  of  the  interviews  none  of  the  adolescents  in  the  control 
group  was  ending  a  romance,  but  a  number  of  the  "suicidal  adolescents" 
had  just  broken  a  romance.  Moreover,  five  of  these  girls  were  either 
pregnant  or  feared  that  they  were  pregnant.  As  the  biographies  revealed, 
pregnancy  inevitably  led  to  a  great  sense  of  isolation.  These  girls  withdrew 
and  were  rejected  by  their  boyfriends.  Usually,  they  were  also  rejected  by 
their  parents  at  this  time  when  they  most  needed  support.  The  suicidal 
adolescents  were  really  in  a  state  of  depression  compared  with  their 
counterparts,  and,  indeed,  as  the  grantees  point  out,  this  seemed  to  have 
been  prompted  by  their  real  experiences  in  life. 


The  Way  They  Saw  It 

Only  38  percent  of  the  suicidal  youngsters  considered  their  childhood 
to  have  been  happy.  But  about  94  percent  of  the  control  group  considered 


Unpublished  transcript. 

333 


childhood    to   have   been  a  happy   time  for  them.   In   describing  the 

biographies,  the  investigators  wrote:3 

"Judging  from  the  verbatim  accounts  of  the  suicide-attempters  in 
the  interviews  as  well  as  the  suicide  notes  left  by  them,  and  notes 
written  by  other  adolescents  outside  our  sample,  the  decision  to 
suicide  was  the  result  of  a  rational,  decision-making  process. 
However,  the  choice  of  death  is  not  based  on  a  desire  to  die,  They 
would,  if  they  could,  choose  to  live,  Death,  in  a  sense,  is  not  chosen 
at  all  but  results  from  the  progressive  failure  of  adaptive  techniques 
to  cope  with  the  problems  of  living,  where  "the  problem"  is  the 
maintenance  of  meaningful  social  relationships.  In  short,  the 
potential  suicide  felt  he  had  no  choice,  i.e.,  death  is  necessary.  It  is 
from  this  recognition  of  necessity  that  his  sense  of  freedom  stems 
and  immediately  preceding  the  act  itself  there  is  often  a  feeling  of 
well-being,  a  cessation  of  all  cares.  This  is  evidenced  in  the  matter- 
of-fact  presentation  found  in  suicide  notes.11 


Profile  of  Problems:  Disruption  at  Home  and  Discipline 

Early  in  childhood  or  adolescence  the  suicidal  youngsters  usually  ex- 
perienced the  break-up  of  their  home.  In  some  cases  this  meant  the 
institutionalization  of  the  child  or  a  family  member.  Many  of  them  were 
placed  in  foster  homes  or  left  with  relatives.  Many  of  them  changed 
schools  and  residences  frequently.  Many  of  these  families  were  very  poor. 
In  some  cases,  the  parents  also  had  been  depressed  and  had  attempted 
suicide.  A  sizable  percent  of  the  suicidal  youngsters  had  either  a  parent, 
relative,  or  close  friend  who  had  attempted  suicide.  Seventy-two  percent 
had  one  or  both  of  their  natural  parents  away  from  home,  eithei  because 
of  divorce,  separation,  or  death.  Most  of  those  living  with  stepparents  felt 
they  didn't  like  the  stepparent.  A  great  many  had  a  parent  who  wfls 
married  several  times.  In  about  62  percent  of  the  cases  both  parents  were 
working.  Half  of  these  families  lived  on  less  than  $3,600  per  year.  The 
background  is  one  of  poverty,  instability,  and  unhappiness. 

The  specific  period  just  preceding  a  suicide  is  characterized  by  a  vicious 
spiral  of  events.  It  may  begin  when  a  parent  feels  unable  to  cope  with 
some  behavior  in  his  or  her  adolescent.  The  parent  begins  to  nag  and  use 
severe  disciplinary  procedures  to  prevent  the  youngster  from  going  out 
He  may  resort  to  physical  punishment.  Parents  of  the  suicidal  adolescents 
*u«*  <-*-e[T  children  would  get  into  less  trouble  if  they  were  watched 


view  of  the  adolescents  (as  revealed  on  a  rating  scale),  withholding  privi- 
leges, fussing,  nagging,  and  whipping  were  considered  the  worst  discipli- 
nary techniques.  The  suicidal  adolescents  and  their  nonsuicidal  counter- 
parts agreed  on  this  rating.  At  the  same  time,  some  of  the  adolescents  felt 
they  would  gladly  forego  undesirable  behavior,  and  their  parents  should 
have  helped  them  to  discourage  this  behavior.  When  the  parents  didn't 
intervene,  the  young  people  took  it  as  a  sign  of  rejection. 

As  the  parent-child  situation  got  worse,  the  parents  grew  frustrated,  and 
the  adolescent  felt  that  his  parents  couldn't  understand  and  were  punish- 
ing him  inappropriately.  The  biographies  revealed  that  this  impasse  led  to 
the  adolescent's  rebellion  or  withdrawal.  This  stage  of  deterioration  usually 
led  to  a  breakdown  of  communication  between  parent  and  child,  in  which 
the  youth's  withdrawal  was  a  consequence.  Essentially,  both  parent  and 
adolescent  would  give  up  and  stop  trying  to  communicate. 

Many  suicidal  adolescents  said  that  they  got  into  the  habit  of  lying  and 
would  simply  withdraw  into  their  rooms,  or  withdraw  into  themselves  in 
order  to  avoid  their  parents  and  conflict. 

School 

A  third  of  the  adolescents  who  had  attempted  suicide  were  out  of 
school  at  the  time.  Either  they  were  ill  because  of  pregnancy  or  because  of 
an  earlier  suicide  attempt.  An  astonishing  number  had  already  attempted 
suicide  in  the  past.  A  quarter  of  these  suicidal  adolescents  had  been  out  of 
school  because  they  were  acting  up  in  class,  had  shown  some  emotional 
instability,  or  had  been  involved  in  fights.  Half  of  them  had  been  truant 
from  school  during  the  last  5  years  because  of  lack  of  interest  or  active 
distaste. 

To  Whom  Do  You  Turn  in  Time  of  Trouble? 

When  asked  to  whom  they  turned  when  they  were  in  trouble,  a  quarter 
of  the  suicidal  adolescents  said  there  was  no  one  to  turn  to.  None  of  the 
control  adolescents  felt  such  isolation.  The  pathos  and  the  loneliness  of 
the  suicidal  adolescent  is  very  dramatically  shown  in  some  of  the  figures. 
Of  the  46  percent  who  reported  their  suicide  attempt  to  other  people,  less 
than  half  reported  it  to  their  parents.  Almost  two-thirds  of  them  talked  to 
people  other  than  family  members.  This  is  particularly  significant  since  88 
percent  of  the  suicide  attempts  occurred  at  home,  very  often  with  the 
parents  in  the  next  room.  In  every  instance,  the  lack  of  communication 
between  family  and  the  child  and  lack  of  communication  with  peers  was  a 
very  important  factor  in  the  period  leading  to  suicide.  On  interview,  these 
suicidal  adolescents  conveyed  the  despairing  sense  that  death  was  the  only 
solution,  there  was  no  other  way  out.  Consider  these  excerpts  from  a 
letter  by  a  17-year-old  Negro  boy  to  his  father.  This  note  was  written  the 
evening  before  he  made  his  second  suicide  attempt: 

"Dear  Father,  I  am  addressing  you  these  few  lines  to  let  you  know 
that  I  am  fine  and  everybody  else  is  and  I  hope  you  are  the  same. 

335 


Daddy,  I  understand  that  I  let  you  down  and  I  let  Mother  down 
the  same  way  when  I  did  that  little  old  thing  [the  suicide  attenip 
that  Wednesday  night.  Daddy,  I  am  sorry  if  I  really  upset  you,  1> 
Daddy  after  I  got  back  I  realized  how  sad  and  bad  you  felt  when 
came  back  to  California. -I  had  lost  my  best  girl  the  week  before 
did  that.  I  had  a  fight  because  some  dude  tried  to  take  advantage  < 
her  when  I  sped  to  the  store,  so  I  came  back  and  I  heard  a  lot  ( 
noise  like  bumping  so  I  run  in  and  there  he  is  trying  to  rape  my  gir 
my  best  one  too.-Daddy  1  tried  as  hard  as  I  could  to  make  it  chcoi 
ful,  but  it  does  get  sad.  Daddy  I  am  up  by  myself.  I've  been  up  & 
night  trying  to  write  you  something  to  cheer  you  up,  because  I  couli 
see  your  heart  breaking  when  you  first  asked  Sam's  wife  if  the; 
would  have  room  and  that  Sunday  Dad,  it  was  hard  but  I  fought  tin 
tears  that  burned  my  eyes  as  we  drove  off  and  Daddy  part  of  ni] 
sickness  when  I  had  taken  an  overdose  I  did  just  want  to  sleep  mysel 
away  because  I  missed  you  Dad. 

"But  when  I  left  I  felt  like  I  had  killed  something  inside  of  yoi 
and  I  knew  you  hated  to  see  me  go,  and  I  hated  to  go,  but  Daddy, 
well,  I  kind  of  missed  Mother  after  I  had  seen  her.  I  miss  yon  niul 
remember  what  you  said,  'settle  down',  but  Daddy  I  tried  so  hard  so 
I  went  and  bought  some  sleeping  pills  and  took  them  so  both  of  you 
could  feel  the  same  tiling."4 

When  an  adolescent  has  retreated  from  family  problems  into  a  love 
affair,  and  then  the  romance  breaks  up  or  culminates  in  pregnancy,  then 
there  is  even  more  isolation  than  before.  A  girl  is  especially  alone  if  her 
boyfriend  disappears  and  she  has  already  alienated  other  friends.  Pnrcnls 
often  become  disillusioned  and  give  up  at  the  time  their  child  needs  help 
the  most.  In  a  letter  to  her  former  boyfriend,  a  desperate  young  girl 
showed  the  lengths  to  which  she  would  go  for  a  social  relationship  and  a 
solution  to  the  problem  of  pregnancy.  She  wrote  on  the  night  of  n  suicide 
attempt.  A  short  excerpt  indicates  the  tragic  sense  of  rejection  and  isola- 
tion. 

"Dear  Bill,  I  want  you  and  I  to  get  an  understanding  about  certain 
things  because  I  think  you  got  the  wrong  impression  of  me  *  *  *  and 
believe  me  it  hurt.  I  knew  all  the  time  you  were  hinting  to  me  I  was 
too  young,  didn't  know  nothing  about  life,  but  you  were  wrong.  I 
know  a  whole  lot  about  life.  I'm  ashamed  of  the  things  I  know  to  be 
so  young.  I  couldn't  tell  you  this  personally,  'cuz  I  couldn't  face 
what  you  might  have  said  and  I  sure  it  would  have  hurt  my  feelings 
badly .^  I'm  two  months  pregnant  by  you.  You  don't  have  to  admit  it, 
1  don  t  care.  You  may  say  anything  you  like.  You  don't  have  to 
worry  about  any  trouble.  It  would  be  a  disgrace  forme  to  let  people 
know  I  threw  myself  on  you  knowing  you  didn't  care  or  feel  anyway 
toward  me.  Don't  worry,  no  one  will  ever  know  my  child's  father.  I 
will  never  mention  you  to  him  or  her  whichever  it  be  "5 


CfT'  J:  D",  and  Jacobs>  Jl  Ado^scents  who  attempt  suicide:  Preliminary 
.  American  Journal  of  Psychiatry,  122(1 1):S,  May  1966. 

Teicner,  J.  D,,  and  Jacobs,  J.  Adolescents  who  attempt  suicide.  American  Journal 
of  Psychiatry,  122(11),  1966. 

336 


Parents  and  Physicians:  Surprised 

Despite  the  history  of  increasing  problems,  the  families  were  inevitably 
hurt  and  surprised  by  the  suicide  attempt.  Parents  and  physicians  who  had 
seen  the  adolescents  would  say  "it  was  so  unexpected."  Actually,  some  46 
percent  of  the  suicide-attempters  had  visited  their  physicians  at  some  time 
before  the  attempt.  Over  half  had  been  treated  for  some  physical  or 
mental  disturbance  during  the  prior  5  years.  A  third  had  some  serious 
physical  complaint,  and  a  third  of  them  had  some  family  member  who 
was  sick  or  had  been  hospitalized.  In  screening  the  adolescents  to  be 
included  in  this  study,  Doctor  Teicher  and  his  associates  examined  over 
100.  In  the  first  30  they  found  1 1  with  duodenal  ulcers. 

In  spite  of  the  long  history  of  problems,  however,  the  physician  and 
mothers  acted  surprised  by  the  suicide  attempts.  While  perhaps  expressing 
some  guilt,  the  mothers  would  deny  that  there  was  anything  in  the  home 
situation  that  would  cause  a  suicide.  The  very  people  who  were  closest  to 
the  suicide-attempters  apparently  failed  to  see  the  progression  of  social 
isolation:  the  problems  with  parents,  with  poverty,  broken  romances, 
excommunication  from  school  or  peers,  especially  in  the  instance  of  preg- 
nancy. Since  these  are  problems  that  most  people  would  be  reticent  to 
discuss  with  others,  adolescents  in  such  predicaments  are  especially  iso- 
lated. 

After  a  period  of  not  communicating,  their  first  suicide  attempt  came 
as  a  surprise  to  parents,  friends,  and  schoolmates.  The  physicans  who  saw 
them  just  after  the  attempt  had  been  taken  off  guard  perhaps  because 
suicidal  people  are  not  easily  distinguished  from  others  with  severe  prob- 
lems. There  seem  to  be  no  simple  and  convenient  ways  of  anticipating  a 
suicidal  attempt.  No  litmus  test  can  determine  who  is  a  potential  suicide. 
Clearly  a  major  reason  that  suicidal  attempts  are  not  warded  off  is  lack  of 
communication  of  the  real  feelings.  The  true  biography  of  the  unhappy 
person  was  not  known  by  anybody  around  him. 


Profiles  for  Prevention 

Adolescence  is  a  time  of  sufficient  duress  for  parents  and  youngsters  as 
new  behavioral  problems  arise.  Moreover,  many  of  the  suicidal  youngsters 
in  the  Los  Angeles  study  also  had  illness  or  mental  illness  in  their  family 
during  the  preceding  5  years.  Doctor  Teicher  and  his  associates  feel  that 
various  sets  of  events  must  be  considered  in  anticipating  suicide.  Among 
them  are  such  factors  as  economic  status,  geographic  mobility,  and  the 
divorce  rate  in  the  home.  These  alone  do  not  predict  suicide.  However, 
these  events  seem  to  occur  at  particular  times  in  the  adolescent's  life  and 
the  timing  may  be  critical.  Along  with  an  escalation  of  behavioral  prob- 
lems, a  youth  who  Is  isolated  from  family  and  peers  may  be  in  danger  of 
trying  suicide. 

It  should  not  be  surprising  to  learn  that  their  parents  also  had  unhappy 
histories.  The  mothers  often  got  married  only  because  they  were  pregnant. 

337 


Some  had  illegitimate  children.  Quite  a  few  suffered  depression  and  were 
depressed  after  giving  birth.  Tin's  was  particularly  notable  among  the 
mothers  of  the  boys  who  had  attempted  suicide.  Many  had  illegitimate 
children  or  had  been  forced  into  marriage  because  of  pregnancy.  Seventy 
percent  of  them  were  separated  or  divorced,  a  good  number  of  them  after 
short-lived  marriages  of  convenience.  Needless  to  say,  a  huge  percentage 
had  suffered  from  economic  deprivation. 


Male  Suicide 

The  number  of  suicides  and  suicide  attempts  among  girls  far  outweighs 
the  number  of  attempts  among  boys;  and  this  has  been  associated  with 
broken  romances,  rejection,  and  unwanted  pregnancy.  In  attempting  (o 
understand  the  male  suicide  attempts,  Doctor  Teicher  and  Dr.  N.L  Mar- 
golin did  a  special  study  of  13  of  the  boys  in  their  group.  They  were 
interviewed  by  one  of  the  authors  after  their  suicide  attempt.  Identical 
questionnaires  about  parent-child  relationships  and  school,  about  adjust- 
ment to  peer  groups  and  career  aspirations  were  given  to  the  boys  find 
their  parents.  Both  took  a  battery  of  psychological  tests  in  addition. 

The  boys  in  the  control  group  also  came  from  broken  homes.  Many  had 
both  parents  working  and  relatives  living  with  the  family.  However,  the 
vignettes  of  the  suicidal  boys  differed  in  that  they  showed  a  repeated 
sequence  of  events  which  the  authors  summarize  in  this  order:  They  had> 
first  of  all>  a  mother  who  was  angry,  depressed,  or  withdrawn,  both  before 
and  after  pregnancy.  Generally  it  was  an  unwanted  pregnancy.  Then,  there 
was  the  loss  of  some  very  significant  person  or  persons  in  the  patient's 
early  life,  usually  the  loss  of  the  father.  There  was  also  a  reversal  of  roles 
with  the  mother.  At  the  time  of  the  suicide  attempt  it  had  seemec!  to  the 
boy  that  the  mother  (or  his  mother-surrogate)  was  also  going  to  leave  his 
life  forever.  During  the  boy's  period  of  distress  his  mother  was  pre- 
occupied with  her  own  depression,  up  to  the  time  of  her  son's  suicide 
attempt. 

An  18-year-old  Mexican-American  boy  is  typical.  His  mother  never 
wanted  him.  She  became  very  over  protective  until  he  was  about  age  12, 
At  age  5  his  semi-alcoholic  father  left  the  home.  At  this  point  he  andliis 
mother  began  to  sliift  around  from  house  to  house,  mostly  living  with  his 
grandmother.  After  the  divorce  he  began  to  get  headaches.  His  mother 
thought  he  missed  his  father.  He  always  felt  rejected,  and  he  made  de- 
pressed statements  such  as:  "I  wish  I  hadn't  been  born."  Then  at  the  age 
of  15  he  was  rejected  by  a  girl.  This  left  him  emotionally  fractured.  H« 
would  get  into  romances  where  he  was  inevitably  hurt  and  depressed.  His 


"I  was  a  very  blind  and  stupid  woman.  I  didn't  realize  what  I  was 
doing  to  Tom,  how  sensitive  and  emotional  he  was.  Well,  time  went 
by  and  Tom  started  to  go  to  parties  and  dating,  not  too  often,  but  he 
had  started  to  have  friendships  on  the  outside.  Soon  after  I  met 
someone  at  work  from  the  same  department  and  we  got  along  real 
well.  He  was  divorced  also.  He  has  a  family  of  three  to  support,  so  we 
have  quite  a  lot  in  common.  The  man  moved  in.  He  liked  my  son  and 
went  out  of  his  way  to  cultivate  him,  but  apparently  things  went 
along  very  well  until  Tom  started  to  complain  that  since  Sam  had 
moved  in  with  us  he  was  nothing  around  the  house  just  in  my  way, 
that  I  didn't  love  him  any  more,  but  that  was  not  true."6 

In  a  pleading  letter  to  the  doctor  she  asked  what  he  could  do  to  undo 
the  damage  she  had  done  her  son  at  an  early  age.  Here  was  the  tragic 
pattern  of  events— the  unhappy  circumstances  around  his  birth,  the  di- 
vorce, his  father's  withdrawal,  infantile  identification  with  the  mother, 
frequent  moves,  repeated  loss  of  peer  relationships,  the  clinging  to  an 
angry  and  depressed  mother,  and,  finally,  the  threatened  loss  of  his 
mother  to  a  new  man.  Case  after  case  revealed  this  kind  of  dependency 
and  frustration  in  the  first  years  of  life.  In  1 1  cases  the  fathers  were 
physically  absent  from  the  home.  In  eight  instances  the  father  had  left 
home  before  the  child  was  6  years  old.  Almost  all  of  these  boys  were 
prevented  from  being  children.  They  were  thrust  into  the  role  of  helping 
their  mothers  either  because  they  were  the  oldest  or  the  only  child.  In 
each  case  there  was  also  a  sense  of  loss  on  the  part  of  the  child,  either 
because  the  mother  and  father  had  just  recently  separated,  because  the 
mother  had  a  serious  illness,  or  because  a  stepfather  had  just  recently  left 
home.  In  one  instance,  the  mother  had  just  recently  married,  and  the  boy 
had  been  left  by  his  girl  friend. 

"On  the  basis  of  our  data  we  find  that  the  male  adolescent  suicide 
attempt  seems  to  have  its  origins  in  the  mother-child  relationships  of 
infancy.  Most  importantly,  these  relationships  revealed  not  only 
early  deprivation,  but  chronic  repeated  separation  threat  or  object 
loss.  This  state  of  affairs  leads  to  continued,  intense,  archaic  identifi- 
cation with  the  mother.  The  lack  of  a  masculine  image  in  the  experi- 
ence of  these  boys  together  with  the  ambivalence  of  the  mothers 
prevents  any  working  through  of  the  Oedipal  phase  of  develop- 
ment."7 

A  helpless  and  dependent  child  needs  his  mother  and  cannot  "allow"  her 
to  be  bad.  He  then  blames  himself  for  anything  wrong  in  the  environment, 
which  allows  him  to  soak  up  the  badness,  as  it  were,  making  things  around' 
him  all  right.  The  investigators  suggest  that  this  situation  eventually  cre- 
ates a  self-destructive  pattern. 

"The  early  and  repeated  separation  trauma  resulting  in  disturb- 
ances in  early  ego  and  superego  development  lay  the  foundation  for 
later  pathological  identification,  and  leave  their  marks  on  character 


^Margolin,  N.   L.,  and  Teicher,  J.  D.  Thirteen  adolescent  male  suicide  attempts. 
Journal  of  the  American  Academy  of  Child  Psychiatry ;  7(2):30I,  1968. 


7Op.cit.  p,312. 


339 


formation  and  personality  development.  As  the  child  enters  adoles- 
cence, the  conflicts  over  separation  intensify  due  to  a  number  of 
concurrent  reasons,  all  of  which  essentially  have  to  do  with  the 
biological  and  psychological  need  to  be  autonomous  from  the 
mother.  The  adolescent  male  tries  to  defend  himself  against  feelings 
of  his  helplessness  in  many  ways.  He  may  regress  to  feelings  of 
omnipotence  and  pseudoindependence  and  seek  challenging,  danger- 
ous situations  such  as  reckless  driving,  motor-cycling,  etc.  He  may 
act  out  antisocially  as  a  defense  to  prevent  loss  of  identity.  How- 
ever, it  seems  that  these  defensive  attempts  cannot  be  maintained 
when  actual  separation  from  the  mother  is  threatened.  This  threat 
can  occur  in  the  form  of  the  mother's  withdrawing  because  of  her 
depression,  her  becoming  interested  in  a  new  husband,  etc,  Also 
significant  is  the  breakup  of  the  adolescent's  romance,  i.e.,  experi- 
encing the  loss  of  a  mother  surrogate.  When  the  mother  becomes 
depressed  and  suicidal,  the  adolescent  perceives  rightly  that  his  very 
existence  is  a  burden  upon  her,  He  acts  as  if  he  were  saying,  'If  I 
destroy  the  bad  part  of  myself,  then  mother  will  live  to  care  for  me.' 
"Internally,  ego  regression  with  splitting  occurs.  The  split-off 
part  of  the  ego,  representing  the  bad  self,  is  rejected  and  persecuted 
by  the  parts  of  the  ego  and  superego  identified  with  the  rejecting 
suicidal  mother.  This  Identification  is  of  great  significance  in  Use 
suicidal  adolescents.  Freud  (1923)  states  that  the  ego,  feeling  hated 
and  unprotected  by  the  superego,  will  let  itself  die,  a  situation  that 
is  similar  to  the  anxiety  in  infantile  separation  from  the  mother."8 

In  these  13  cases,  the  boys  professed  to  love  their  nagging  and  ambiva- 
lent mothers,  They  did  not  necessarily  feel  they  were  loved,  but  because 
of  an  infantile  dependence,  the  mother's  depression,  anger,  withdrawal,  find 
disapproval  had  a  very  devasting  effect  upon  them.  In  many  instances,  the 
mother  also  had  suicidal  thoughts,  and  the  boys  identified  with  their 
mother's  depressed  and  suicidal  state.  Interestingly  enough,  the  suicidal 
girls  described  their  mothers  in  uniformly  glowing  and  idealized  terms  and 
denied  any  flaws,  despite  the  fact  that  their  mothers  were  often  very 
hostile. 

"The  suicide  attempt  is  an  overdetermined  symptom  and  whether 
it  is  an  attention-getting  or  an  attempt  to  die  it  is  always  serious.  It  is 
an  effort  to  solve  a  chronic  problem,  living;  a  plea  for  help;  an 
expression  of  rage  and  hostility;  and  at  times  a  symbolic  reunion 
with  the  pre-Oedipal  mother  or  father."9 

Therapy 

In  many  ways  the  therapist  in  the  hospital  has  proven  to  be  the  lifeline 
of  these  youngsters.  He  maintains  his  contact  with  the  suicide  attempters 
from  the  beginning  of  consultation  until  final  rehabilitation  or  referral. 


"Op.cit.  pp.  312-313. 

9Teicher,  J.  D.  The  treatment  of  the  suicidal  adolescent-the  lifeline  approach. 
Proceedings  of  the  JV  World  Congress  of  Psychiatry,  p.  749,  Madrid,  September  1966, 
Excerpta  Medica  International  Congress  Series  No.  150. 

340 


When  they  are  first  brought  to  the  hospital  they  are  shaken,  anxious, 
depressed,  insecure,  guilty,  and  apprehensive  because  of  the  anger  and 
hurt  that  they've  caused.  They  feel  terribly  alone,  and  this  is  probably 
their  worst  agony.  Usually  the  mother  has  been  angry  and  sometimes 
guilty;  her  next  reaction  is  usually  hostile  and  she  will  defend  herself  with 
great  denial.  The  father,  or  more  usually  the  stepfather,  would  consider 
the  suicide  attempt  a  bother  and  show  little  concern.  Doctor  Teicher 
recommends  that  suicidal  adolescents  should  be  hospitalized,  if  only 
briefly,  and  placed  in  a  ward  where  there  are  other  adolescent  patients  to 
offer  warmth,  support,  and  understanding.  In  many  instances  the  patients 
of  this  study  didn't  want  to  leave  the  hospital,  and  they  would  cling  to  the 
staff  and  other  patients.  Adolescents  will  often  talk  about  the  precipi- 
tating events,  such  as  their  parents'  refusal  to  let  them  go  out,  or  a  broken 
romance.  The  rejection  by  a  boyfriend  or  a  girlfriend  is  a  most  common 
precipitating  factor,  but  this  would  be  taken  in  stride  as  an  unhappy 
experience  if  there  had  been  some  positive  experiences  earlier  in  life.  The 
role  of  the  therapist  as  seen  by  the  investigators  is  that  of  a  person  who 
provides  understanding  and  love.  Slowly  the  therapist  can  guide  a  young 
person  to  cope  with  his  conflicts  and  communicate  with  his  family.  Mean- 
while he  offers  support  and  is  always  available  so  that  the  adolescent 
doesn't  feel  so  lonely  and  isolated. 

From  this  study  one  may  clearly  see  that  youth,  itself,  is  no  antidote  to 
a  hostile  environment.  The  old  myth  that  all  suicide  attempts  are  impul- 
sive and  irrational  is  forever  banished,  and  in  this  study  one  can  see  how 
an  accumulation  of  adverse  factors  at  a  critical  period  shapes  the  biograph- 
ical profile  of  the  potential  adolescent  suicide.  This  profile  might  be  used 
in  further  studies  to  predict  and  prevent  suicide  attempts. 

This  brief  research  has  already  shown  that  no  simple  correlations  be- 
tween life  events  can  predict  suicidal  despair  in  a  young  person.  Yet  young 
people-in  shockingly  vast  numbers-are  miserable  enough,  and  lonely 
enough  that  they  are  brought  to  hospitals  by  the  tens  of  thousands  each 
year,  after  attempting  to  kill  themselves,  often  in  a  room  right  next  to 
their  parents. 

Further  research  in  this  area  has  implications  beyond  suicide  preven- 
tion. The  development  of  biographical  profiles  may  yield  techniques 
whereby  informed  doctors,  social  workers,  and  school  personnel  might 
spot  the  precarious  young  person  in  time  to  obtain  therapy  for  him. 
However,  the  import  of  this  research  is  broader  in  its  implications.  It 
begins  to  fold  back  the  curtains  upon  the  circumstances  and  the  timing 
that  weaken  an  individual  to  the  stresses  of  life  and  alienate  him  from  all 
of  those  who  might  help  him.  The  chain  of  misery  seems  to  pass  from 
one  generation  to  the  next,  and  in  each  case  privation  plays  its  part. 
Moreover,  the  relations  of  family  members  show  a  psychodynamics  that 
produces  instability  and  separation  instead  of  cohesion  and  mutual  help. 
Adolescence  can  be  an  especially  creative  and  exciting  time  of  life.  In  this 
particular  era,  adolescents  are  having  an  ever-increasing  impact  upon 
society— they  have  changed  the  entire  genre  of  popular  music,  for 
example-but  exceedingly  great  numbers  of  adolescents  are  having  the 

341 


opposite  experience.  Suicide  prevention  studies  among  the  most  unhappy 
of  these  people  may  give  considerable  insight  into  what  it  takes  to  deflect 
an  entire  life  from  misery  toward  productiveness  and  participation. 


Research  Grant:   MH  1432 

Date  of  Interview.   September  1968 

References: 

Jacobs,  J  ,  and  Teicher,  J.  D.  Broken  homes  and  social  isolation  in  attempted  suicides 
of  adolescents.  International  Journal  of  Social  Psychiatry,  I3(2):140-149,  1967. 

Jacobziner,  H.  Attempted  suicide  in  adolescence,  Journal  of  the  American  Medical 
Association,  191(7):1 1-14,  1965. 

Margolint  N.  L.>  and  Teicher,  J  D.  Thirteen  adolescent  male  suicide  attempts.  Journal 
of  Child  Psychiatry,  7(2):296-314,  April  1968. 

Teicher,  J.  D.  The  treatment  of  the  suicidal  adolescent-the  lifeline  approach.  Proceed- 
ings of  the  IV  World  Congress  of  Psychiatry,  Madrid,  September  1966,  Exccrpla 
Medica  International  Congress  Series  No.  150. 

Teicher,  J.  D.,  and  Jacobs,  J,  Adolescents  who  attempt  suicide:  preliminary  findings. 
American  Journal  of  Psychiatry,  122(11).  May  1966. 

The  physician  and  the  adolescent  suicide  attempter.  Journal  of  School  Health* 

36(9):406-415,  November  1966. 


342 


Investigator: 

Elizabeth  Elmer,  M.S.S. 
Assistant  Professor  of  Social  Case  Work 

School  of  Medicine 
University  of  Pittsburgh 
Pittsburgh,  Pennsylvania 

Co- Investigators: 

Grace  Gregg,  M.D. 

Byron  Wright,  M.A. 

John  B.  Reinhart,  M.D. 

Contributors: 

Thomas  McHenry,  M.D. 

Bertram  Girdony,  M.D. 

Paul  Geisel,  Ph.D. 

Prepared  by: 

Clarissa  Wittenberg 


Historically,  the  terrible  toll  taken  by  childhood  illness  and  industrial 
accidents  overshadowed  the  risk  of  children  being  injured  by  their  parents. 
At  one  time  children  were  believed  to  be  in  the  grip  of  the  devil  because 
they  had  been  conceived  in  sin,  and  harsh  punishment  was  thought 
necessary  to  save  them.  Parents  "owned"  children  and  almost  any 
punishment  was  considered  legitimate.  As  our  concepts  of  child 
development  have  become  more  sophisticated  and  our  understanding  of 
learning  and  discipline  has  advanced,  harsh  punishment  has  become  less 
and  less  acceptable.  Consequently  the  parent  who  beats  his  child  is  an 
object  of  censure.  Today,  we  hold  parents  responsible  for  the  well-being 
of  children,  and,  therefore,  the  malnourished  and  medically  neglected 
child  becomes  a  subject  of  concern.  Recognition  that  parents  do  abuse 
their  children  has  grown,  and  hospitals  and  doctors  are  increasingly  aware 
of  the  problem.  Studies  have  been  done  to  help  define  the  problem  and  its 
dimensions,  and  to  record  the  effects  of  abuse  on  children. 

Children's  Hospital  of  Pittsburgh  is  located  in  a  large  low-income 
district  of  the  inner  city  and,  like  many  other  city  hospitals,  has  an  active 
emergency  ward  where  many  injured  children  are  brought.  Some  of  these 
children  have  multiple  bone  injuries.  Early  in  the  1960*s  the  staff  began  to 
systematically  study  the  possibility  of  abuse  in  these  cases.  A  research 
team  headed  by  Miss  Elizabeth  Elmer  began,  in  1962,  a  study  of  50 
families  with  children  suffering  from  bone  injuries  who  had  been  admitted 

343 


to  the  hospital  over  the  previous  13  yeais.  This  was  a  follow-up  study  lo 
determine  what  happened  to  these  children  after  their  initial  admission  to 
the  hospital.  A  second  study  followed  which  focused  on  infant  accidents, 
and  compared  infants  and  families  where  accidental  injury  had  occuned 
with  those  where  neglect  or  abuse  was  present. 

The  problem  of  diagnosing  abuse  was  attacked  in  the  first  study.  Oases 
were  selected  for  the  follow-up  study  on  the  basis  of  their  hospital 
admission  record.  Although  abuse  is  a  complicated  subject  involving  both 
social  and  medical  problems,  the  criterion  of  multiple  bone  injuries  was 
selected  for  the  purpose  of  a  less  controversial  diagnosis.  The  family 
history  was  then  examined  and  the  families  judged  to  be  either  abusive  or 
nonabusive;  those  who  could  not  be  placed  clearly  in  either  group  wore 
considered  unclassified. 

TVie  first  study  showed  clearly  that  these  children  are  in  scrtous 
jeopardy,  that  many  die  and  many  become  severely  retarded  ami/or 
crippled  and  spend  their  lives  in  institutions.  The  first  Study  of  Fifty 
Families  resulted  in  an  examination  of  what  constitutes  abuse.  An 
examination  of  the  "failure  to  thrive"  child  was  begun  and  the  rote  of 
accidental  injuries  noted.  The  second  study  focused  on  small  babies  not 
yet  capable  of  getting  into  trouble  on  their  own,  thus  illuminating  the  role 
of  the  parents  in  such  accidents. 

Both  studies  resulted  in  an  examination  of  the  theories  and  accepted 
ideas  surrounding  tliis  issue.  For  instance,  the  working  mother,  commonly 
felt  to  contribute  to  child  abuse,  did  not  appear  to  be  important.  Neither 
were  these  children  typically  abused  by  extramarital  partners  or 
non-related  figures;  there  was  no  "wicked  stepmother"  syndrome.  Wliile 
many  parents  were  found  to  have  serious  emotional  problems,  few  wore 
mistreating  the  children  for  bizarre  or  extemely  sadistic  reasons.  These 
children  were  rarely  abused  "coldly,"  Few  of  the  parents  were  "bud" 
parents  and  total  failures;  most  stayed  with  their  families  and  eventually 
exhibited  some  success  with  their  children.  Neither  did  the  parents 
typically  injure  all  of  their  children.  Abuse  has  been  found  to  be  a 
phenomenon  related  to  the  child-bearing  period  of  the  mother,  and  often 
the  mother  has  been  uninformed  about  contraception. 


THE  FIFTY  FAMILY  STUDY 

a  pediatrician  selected  50  former  patients  for  the 

'«ln^o/1. 

iy  x-ray  film,  indicating  the  occurrence 
rt  %  in  conjunction  with  - 

that  might  account  for  the 


ui  assault  or  gross  neglect,  or  the  absence  of  a  history 
showing  convincingly  that  the  injuries  were  accidental  or  attributable  to 
an  unusually  traumatic  delivery,  A  small  group  of  children  do  suffer 
undiagnosed  fractures  at  birth. 


344 


The  Final  group  was  equally  divided  between  male  and  female  subjects, 
of  whom  36  were  white  and  14  Negro.  This  racial  distribution 
approximated  that  of  the  hospital's  clientele.  A  number  of  the  children 
had  come  to  the  hospital  for  other  complaints,  and  bone  injuries  had  been 
discovered  in  the  course  of  routine  examinations. 

The  majority  of  subjects  had  been  young  babies  at  the  time  of  their 
admission.  Seventeen  were  under  3  months  of  age  when  multiple  injuries 
were  found.  Nine  were  between  3  and  9  months  of  age,  This  is  in  contrast 
to  the  curve  for  childhood  accidents  where  the  incidence  rate  for 
accidents  is  minimal  below  the  age  of  9  months.  It  then  begins  a  sharp 
climb,  reaching  a  peak  between  2  and  3  years  when  it  begins  to  level  off. 

Fewer  than  50  families  were  actually  interviewed  -  due  to  deaths, 
institutional  placements,  and  refusals.  Only  families  who  still  had  their 
children  were  interviewed.  Six  families  refused  to  cooperate.  Thirty-one  of 
the  children  in  the  original  group,  plus  two  siblings  found  to  have  bone 
injuries,  added  up  to  a  total  of  33  children  studied  and  31  mothers 
interviewed.  Seven  were  foster  mothers  and  one  an  adoptive  mother. 
Essentially  the  families  were  told  that  the  object  of  the  study  was  to 
examine  the  hospital's  treatment  of  patients,  and  an  attempt  was  made  to 
avoid  focusing  on  the  suspected  episodes  of  abuse  in  order  to  minimize 
suspicion  and  distortion.  It  is  of  interest  that  the  noncooperative  parents 
were  in  general  better  educated  than  the  rest  of  the  group.  They  may  have 
been  more  suspicious  of  the  hospital's  motives  or  more  guilty  about  their 
own  behavior. 

Information  was  accumulated  from  hospital  records,  current 
examinations,  home  visits,  and  interviews  with  the  mothers.  It  was 
initially  anticipated  that  the  fathers  would  not  be  available  for  interviews. 
Fathers  were  not  interviewed  formally,  and  it  was  felt  that  potentially 
information  from  the  fathers  would  have  been  of  value. 

Each  of  the  children  was  given  a  current  examination  which  included  a 
complete  pediatric  evaluation,  psychological  testing,  a  psychiatric 
interview,  a  hearing  test,  and  an  x-ray  survey  of  skull,  long  bones,  chest, 
pelvis,  and  spine— with  special  attention  to  the  sites  of  old  bone  injuries. 

On  the  basis  of  all  this  information,  the  children  were  divided  into  three 
groups.  Twenty-two  were  considered  abused,  four  nonabused,  and  seven 
unclassified.  Nonabused  children  were  those  whose  early  bone  injuries  had 
a  plausible  explanation  other  than  assault  by  an  adult.  If  agreement  could 
not  be  reached  as  to  the  cause  of  the  injury,  the  child  was  considered 
unclassified.  For  example,  one  such  child  had  a  record  of  birth  injuries 
and  a  hospital  admission  at  3  months  with  fresh  fractures,  but  no  account 
could  be  obtained  of  abuse  or  accident.  Unclassified  families,  then,  while 
not  labeled  abusive  cannot  be  considered  nonabusive  either. 

Almost  all  of  the  families  struggled  to  live  on  low  incomes.  Most  had 
less  than  a  high  school  education  and  correspondingly  few  job 
opportunities.  In  most  cases  the  families  had  three  or  four  children.  About 
a  quarter  of  the  families  in  this  study  were  on  welfare;  however,  none  of 
the  nonabusive  families  was,  The  families  lived  in  substandard,  but  not  the 
worst,  housing.  Most  lived  in  private  dwellings  or  apartments,  but  none  in 

345 


trailers  or  rooming  houses.  Many  of  the  families  kept  their  homes  in  fai 
good  condition  and  the  mothers  tended  to  be  good  housekeepers.  Physi 
squalor  was  not  characteristic  of  this  group. 

The  study  families,  particularly  the  abusive  ones,  suffered  from  man 
stress.  Many  couples  had  been  separated   and  reconciled  many  tim 
without  coming  to  any  real  resolution  of  their  problems  or  differenci 
The  abusive  families  tended  to  have  more  quarreling  and  drinking  than  tl 
others.  Several  abusive  mothers  expressed  fear  of  their  husbands,  and  tl 
investigators  thought  that  in  general  their  fear  was  well  justified.  Oi 
fathei,  for  instance,  had  a  prison  record  for  murder;  another  was  observt 
to  blow  cigarette  ashes  in  his  baby's  eyes  and  then  to  knock  the  child 
head  against  a  post.  It  is  possible  that  mothers  with  poor  self-control  ten 
to  be  attracted  to  men  with  similar  problems,  or  that  the  mothers  war 
the   fathers  to  appear  in  a  bad  light  so  as  to  appear  sympathetic  b 
comparison. 

For  disciplinary  measures  most  of  these  families  relied  on  physia 
means  of  control.  Whipping  and  spanking  were  the  most  commonly  nsci 
methods  of  discipline;  scolding,  withdrawal  of  privileges,  shaming,  am 
shaking  were  also  common.  Reasoning  with  a  child  or  avoidance  of  tin 
conflict  were  methods  almost  never  used,  These  parents  tended  to  set 
even  small  infants  as  needing  discipline  and  as  consciously  and  deliberately 
misbehaving.  It  was  rare  for  anyone  other  than  the  mother,  or  the  mothei 
and  the  father  together,  to  discipline  a  child  -  and  very  unusual  for  th( 
father  to  deal  with  the  children  by  himself. 

The  nonatmsive  families  tended  to  use  a  few  types  of  punishments 
consistently,  while  some  of  the  abusive  families  used  a  broad  range  of 
disciplinary  measures  that  they  were  searching  for  some  effective  way  to 
manage  their  children. 

Mothers  who  abused  their  children  felt  very  negatively  toward  the  child 
who  had  been  injured.  It  is  not  known  if  they  felt  this  way  about  all  their 
children  or  only  the  one  who  was  abused.  In  one  exceptional  case,  the 
mother  expressed  sympathy  for  the  child  who  had  been  abused  by  her 
husband. 

The  abusive  mothers  appeared  to  have  more  emotional  problems  of 
greater  severity  than  the  nonabusive  ones".  Depression  was  common  with 
about  half  of  the  abusive  mothers  troubled  by  difficulties  in  eating  or 
sleeping  and  having  a  tendency  toward  crying  spells.  The  nonabusive 
mothers,  in  general,  had  fewer  and  milder  symptoms. 

Several  of  the  abusive  mothers  admitted  to  uncontrollable  actions  in 
the  past  —  including  physical  aggression  against  other  women;  sexual 
promiscuity;  and  secret,  compulsive  spending.  These  mothers,  who 
themselves  had  serious  problems  of  control,  admitted  being  afraid  of  their 
husbands  as  well.  By  their  own  reports,  more  of  the  abusive  mothers  than 
the  nonabusive  were  easily  irritated. 

The  abusive  mothers  were  lonely  people,  often  with  no  place  to  escape 
from  the  pressures  of  home  and  children.  In  many  cases  they  had  poor 
relationships  with  their  own  parents.  There  were  no  friends  or  relatives  to 

346 


help.  It  was  noted  that  the  mothers  actively  discomaged  friendships,  and 
did  not  join  even  relatively  impersonal  groups  such  as  the  PTA. 

Child  abuse  is  a  family  affair,  however,  and  regardless  of  the  identity  of 
the  abuser,  the  rest  of  the  family  participates.  The  other  parent  is  involved 
by  virtue  of  lack  of  interference  or  tacit  approval.  In  many  cases  siblings 
may  have  injured  the  child,  but  again  the  responsibility  must  rest  with  the 
parents.  The  family  dynamics  are  important  in  these  situations. 

The  following  case  history  illustrates  the  type  of  family  problems  that 
surround  child  abuse: 

A  19-year  old  mother  brought  a  three -mo  nth -old  baby,  her  third,  into 
the  hospital.  The  baby  was  wearing  a  cast,  and  his  weight  including  that  of 
the  cast  was  10  pounds.  He  had  had  a  birth  weight  of  5  pounds,  3  ounces. 
His  x-ray  showed  that  he  had  an  old  fracture  of  the  skull,  an  injury  to  his 
shoulder,  fracture  of  the  left  arm,  multiple  injuries  to  knees,  ankles,  and 
long  bones  of  both  legs.  In  addition,  he  had  a  bulging  fontanel  suggesting 
subdural  hematoma. 

The  mother  expressed  her  horror  that  eveiy  time  she  picked  up  the 
baby  he  appeared  to  have  something  else  wrong  with  him.  The  child  had 
been  in  another  hospital  at  six  weeks  of  age  when  he  had  been  injured 
falling  off  a  bed  onto  a  concrete  floor.  When  the  emergency  room  doctor 
saw  the  baby,  he  wanted  the  police  called  as  he  thought  it  obvious  that 
the  child  had  been  beaten.  The  baby  needed  two  subdural  taps  at  that 
time. 

The  mother's  explanation  was  that  she  had  put  the  baby  in  the  middle 
of  a  double  bed  while  she  went  to  another  room  to  wash  his  crib.  A 
14-month-old  sibling  was  in  the  room  with  the  baby.  She  heard  a  thump 
and  thought  toys  had  been  dropped,  and  then  ran  to  find  the  baby  on  the 
floor.  She  assumed  that  he  had  "scooted  off"  the  bed.  The  father  was 
critical  of  the  mother  for  not  watching  the  baby. 

The  mother  had  been  a  favorite  child  and  had  attended  church 
faithfully.  Her  family  had  had  ambitions  for  her  to  get  a  good  education. 
At  sixteen,  however,  she  became  pregnant  and  was  disowned  by  her 
parents.  The  minister  of  her  church  was  also  very  critical  of  her.  The  baby 
was  born  after  a  six-month  gestation  period  and  died  after  three  days. 
During  this  crisis  the  mother  was  alone  as  none  of  her  family  came  to  see 
her.  She  married  the  father,  and  became  pregnant  again  and  had  a  little 
boy.  When  he  was  three  months  old,  she  became  pregnant  again  and 
delivered  the  baby  who  was  the  patient.  This  added  up  to  three  births 
within  22  months.  Two  children  had  been  premature.  In  addition,  her 
parents  separated  and  blamed  their  troubles  upon  her  "disgrace." 


The  Children 

Most  of  the  children  were  quite  young  at  the  tiro 
admission  to  the  hospital.  This  study  has  shown  that  mar 
suffer  grave  and  irreversible  damage,  but  also  that  som 


this  early  abuse,  and  leach  a  phase  where  their  parents  can  successfully 
care  for  them  and  they  can  attain  a  reasonably  good  physical  condition. 

Eight  of  the  50  children  had  died  by  the  time  of  this  study.  Most  had 
been  under  five  months  of  age  at  the  time  of  death;  two  had  been  slain  by 
their  mothers.  Five  children  were  in  State  institutions  for  the  retaidcd. 
Many,  who  were  in  basically  good  health,  had  scars  or  deformities,  but 
considering  that  they  had  been  at  the  point  of  death  and  had  suffered  very 
serious  injuries  they  were  quite  well-recovered.  One  child  was  suffering 
from  malnutrition,  and  several  had  organic  brain  defects.  A  large  number 
of  children  were  observed  to  show  signs  of  upper  motor  neurone  disease, 
as  manifested  by  hyperactive  tendon  reflexes  as  well  as  abnormal  plantar 
reflexes.  A  few  children  had  signs  of  cranial  nerve  involvement  manifested 
by  strabismus  and  nystagmus.  These  signs  appeared  to  be  related  to  injury 
in  all  of  the  children  born  at  full  leim,  except  in  the  case  of  one  who  was 
jaundiced  at  birth  and  had  had  convulsions  prior  to  the  injury. 

In  the  premature  children  with  signs  of  neurological  damage,  the  effect 
of  prematurity  itself  cannot  be  discounted.  Only  two  of  the  prematures 
were  known  to  have  had  head  trauma  and  symptomatic  convulsions.  In 
one  child,  prematurity  was  the  only  known  condition  that  could  account 
for  central  nervous  system  damage.  The  abused  children  had  twice  (lie 
incidence  of  neurological  signs  as  was  true  of  the  rest  of  the  group. 

The  investigators  found  that  two  of  the  children  had  been  injured  in 
substitute  homes.  In  one  case  the  substitute  home  was  arranged  through 
an  informal  agreement  between  the  natural  and  the  foster  parents,  ami  in 
the  other  an  adoption  agency  chose  the  home  for  an  infant  who  was  born 
out  of  wedlock.  The  latter  child  was  subsequently  moved  to  another 
foster  home.  In  all,  1 1  children  in  the  study  were  moved  to  snbstiliilc 
homes  for  their  own  protection,  following  the  abusive  incidents. 


Birth 

Histories  were  obtained  from  the  mother,  and  other  available  sources 
such  as  hospital  records.  It  was  found  that  about  a  third  of  the  cMhlm 
weighed  less  than  5,5  pounds  at  birth,  indicating  prematurity.  As  Hie 
national  figure  for  prematurity  is  8  percent,  the  percentage  in  this  study  is 
extraordinarily  high.  It  is  known  that  birth  weight  varies  by  race  and  by 
socioecnomic  status.  The  national  rates  are  7  percent  prematurity  for 
whites  and  1 2  percent  for  nonwhites.  In  this  study,  however,  the  higher 
percentages  of  low  birth  weight  occurred  among  the  white  families:  8  of 
the  24  white  and  only  2  of  the  8  Negro  children  ha'd  birth  weights  of  less 
than  5.5  pounds.  Although  the  significance  of  the  large  number  of 
premature  babies  is  not  known,  one  possible  explanation  is  that  prem- 
ature infants,  because  of  their  incomplete  development  at  birth,  are 
more  vulnerable  to  bone  injuries  than  full-term  infants.  A  pedialric 
radiologist,  Dr.  John  Caffey,  is  of  the  opinion  that  there  is  more 
vulnerability  in  the  first  few  weeks.  The  bones  of  a  premature  baby  may 

348 


be  injured  even  with  normal  handling,  for  instance,  during  diapering. 
However,  when  chronological  age  plus  the  number  of  weeks  of 
prematurity  equals  nine  months,  Dr.  Caffey  obseives  that  vulnerability  to 
bone  injury  becomes  that  of  any  full-term  newborn. 

The  median  age  of  the  premature  children  in  this  study  was  1 1  months 
at  the  time  of  hospital  admission.  This  would  indicate  that  their  injuries 
were  not  due  to  immature  bone  development,  but  to  othei  factors.  Of  the 
21  abused  children  whose  birth  weight  was  known,  seven  were  premature; 
none  of  the  nonabused  children  were  premature.  It  is  known  that 
premature  babies  aie  more  difficult  to  caie  for  than  full-term  ones;  they 
may  be  more  irritable  and  cry  more  due  to  their  immature  nervous 
systems.  The  mothers  may  be  more  apprehensive  about  picking  them  up 
because  they  are  so  tiny.  In  addition,  the  emergency  situation  that  so 
often  surrounds  premature  birth  may  be  a  serious  strain  on  an  already 
easily  upset  mother.  Preparations  for  births  are  often  incomplete  when  a 
prematwe  baby  arrives,  and  for  a  family  with  only  marginal  resources  the 
strain  can  be  severe. 

Negroes,  who  often  had  extended  families  or  else  lived  in  overcrowded 
housing  where  other  women  were  available,  seemed  to  cope  with  the 
strains  of  prematurity  better,  with  relatively  fewer  combinations  of 
prematurity  and  abuse. 

There  is  another  issue,  loo,  that  must  be  considered:  the  more  subtle 
problem  of  the  mother's  condition  during  pregnancy.  A  woman  who  is 
unhappy  about  herself,  her  marriage,  her  pregnancy,  or  her  other  children 
may  take  inadequate  care  of  herself  or  be  too  overwhelmed  to  obtain 
help.  In  many  cases  these  mothers  may  not  even  seek  prenatal  care. 

Other  questions  arise:  For  instance,  what  causes  one  family  to  zealously 
protect,  or  even  overprotect,  a  premature  infant,  and  another  family  to 
abuse  such  an  infant.  Why,  if  a  couple  with  abusive  tendencies  has  other 
children,  is  the  premature  child  selected  for  abuse? 

Conditions  at  Time  of  Original  Admission 

At  the  time  of  admission  to  the  hospital  there  was  no  difference 
between  the  chief  complaints  of  the  abused  children  and  the  others.  The 
majority  were  brought  to  the  hospital  because  of  limitation  of  motion  or 
pain  in  an  extremity.  The  next  most  common  complaint  was  convulsions, 
then  "failure  to  thrive,"  and  gastric  symptoms.  Convulsions  and  subdural 
liematomas,  physical  conditions  that  are  often  associated  with  brain 
damage,  were  diagnosed  in  eight  children  upon  admission.  Surgical 
procedures  connected  with  subdural  hematomas  were  necessary  in  seven 
cases.  Two  other  children  required  orthopedic  surgery  due  to  bone 
injuries.  One-third  of  the  group  had  previous  hospital  admissions. 

Records  of  growth  show  that  poor  growth  and  abuse  are  not  always 
associated.  However,  many  of  the  children  showed  an  improvement  in 
appetite  and  growth  while  they  were  in  the  hospital. 
/ 

349 


Condition  at  Time  of  Study 
Retardation 

Forty-five  percent  of  the  entire  study  group  had  IQs  under  80.  Twelve 
of  the  22  abused  children  and  none  of  the  nonabused  children  fell  in  this 
low  IQ  group.  This  is  more  striking  when  one  realizes  that  this  group  does 
not  include  five  of  the  original  children  who  were  placed  in  State  institu- 
tions for  the  letardecl.  Only  children  still  at  home  were  included  in  the 
study.  The  investigators  stress  that  they  have  no  way  of  knowing  what  was 
cause  and  what  was  effect  in  this  relationship  between  abuse  and  retalia- 
tion. Neurological  impairment  is  important  in  retardation,  and  many  of 
these  children  showed  such  signs.  In  addition,  many  had  histories  of  poor 
early  growth,  a  condition  thought  to  be  associated  with  later  mental  re- 
tardation. 

Speech  problems,  which  are  often  associated  with  both  emotional 
difficulties  and  mental  retardation,  were  found  in  this  study  to  be  more 
closely  related  to  mental  retardation  than  to  emotional  problems. 


Emotional  Characteristics 

The  abused  children  had  marked  difficulty  in  impulse  control  ns 
compared  with  others  in  the  study.  Many  of  the  children,  regardless  of 
their  classification,  had  poor  self-concepts.  Even  the  nonabused  childien 
had  suffered  serious  injuries,  pain,  and  traumatic  experiences  at  an  early 
age.  Most  had  scars  or  physical  deformities.  It  is  not  difficult  to 
understand  that  they  might  view  themselves  poorly  or  feel  inferior, 
especially  if  the  parents  had  not  been  able  to  help  them  in  a  sensitive  way, 

Eight  of  the  abused  children  had  difficulty  in  controlling  anger,  ami 
either  had  outbursts,  of  rage  or  serious  inhibition  of  negative  feelings, 
manifested  by  very  apathetic  responses. 


General  Functioning 

The  abused  children  who  remained  in  the  same  environments  had  a 
substantially  greater  number  of  problems  than  the  nonabused  children. 
Eight  of  these  were  retarded.  The  unclassified  children  had  more  general 
problems  than  the  abused  children  who  had  been  moved  to  foster  homes. 
Seven  children,  whose  physical  development  had  been  poor  at  the  time  of 
hospital  admission  and  who  had  been  moved  from  the  home,  had  achieved 
an  average  level  by  the  time  of  the  study.  To  emphasize  the  importance  of 
the  home  environment,  two  children  who  remained  in  the  same  poor 
homes  showed  average  development  on  admission  but  below  average 
development  at  the  time  of  the  study. 


Families  at  the  Time  of  Abuse 

The  abusive  families  by  and  large  lived  in  far  more  difficult 
circumstances  than  did  the  nonabusive  families.  However,  all  these 
families  had  often  lived  under  stress,  and  for  some  reason  abuse  was  not  a 
constant  process.  Rather,  it  breaks  out  and  then  abates.  In  many  cases,  the 
sex  or  oidinal  position  of  the  abused  child  had  a  special  significance  for 
the  abusive  parent.  One  child  was  a  second  girl,  as  her  mother  had  been, 
and  both  were  family  scapegoats.  In  another  family,  the  two  girls  were 
severely  abused  by  the  mother,  but  never  the  boys. 

The  birth  of  a  sibling  less  than  one  year  before  or  nine  months  after  the 
incident  of  abuse  was  found  to  be  important.  Nine  of  the  abusive  mothers 
were  pregnant  at  the  time  of  the  abused  child's  hospital  admission,  one 
abusive  mother  had  miscarried  just  before  the  child  was  admitted,  and  two 
others  had  borne  an  infant  other  than  the  patient  during  the  year  piior  to 
admission.  In  only  one  of  the  other  1 1  families,  unclassified  and 
nonabusive  taken  together,  was  there  an  interval  of  less  than  one  year 
between  the  injured  child's  admission  and  the  birth  of  a  sibling. 

The  investigators  found  that  the  connection  between  abuse  and  the 
burdens  of  pregnancy  and  child-rearing  is  clear  and  important.  They  cite 
the  theories  of  Bibring,  who  identified  pregnancy  as  a  biologically 
determined  maturational  crisis  that  is  not  always  resolved  with  the  birth 
of  the  baby,  but  usually  continues  for  some  time,  even  in  the  most 
auspicious  circumstances.  The  investigators  point  out  that  these  families 
abuse  their  children  primarily  during  the  child-bearing  phase  of  marriage, 
Later  they  appear  to  cope  in  a  better  fashion.  //  was  found  that  those 
families  who  had  successfully  begun  to  use  contraception  were  able  to 
recover  from  their  previous  strain  and  to  stop  venting  their  feelings  of 
frustration  and  rage  upon  their  children, 

Several  of  the  mothers  who  were  abusive  were  quite  disturbed,  and 
some  were  under  psychiatric  care.  In  three  cases  the  fathers  were  clearly 
very  disturbed  or  antisocial. 


Substitute  Care 

A  change  in  environment  often  saves  the  life  of  a  child  who  has  been 
assaulted.  Still,  some  children  were  abused  while  in  foster  care.  It  was 
found  that  foster  parents  who  voluntarily  took  children  who  were  injured, 
neurotic,  or  retarded  often  had  an  unhealthy  need  to  have  children  who 
were  excessively  dependent  upon  them.  Furthermore,  while  much  good 
can  be  accomplished,  even  the  best  foster  or  adoptive  parents  cannot  undo 
irrevocable  damage  already  done  to  the  child. 

Some  foster  children,  due  to  their  previous  abuse,  have  severe 
difficulties  even  after  the  original  crisis  is  resolved.  These  are  troubled 
children  and  symptoms  can  appear  long  after  the  original  trauma.  In 
addition,  the  protective  care  so  helpful  in  the  beginning  can  cause  rebellion 
later  if  the  foster  parents  are  unable  to  modify  their  methods  in 

351 


accordance  with  the  changing  developmental  needs  of  the  growing  child, 
However,  most  of  the  children  in  placement  showed  marked  improvement 
in  their  physical  health. 


INFANT  ACCIDENT  STUDY 

The  majority  of  the  abused  children  in  the  Fifty  Families  Study  hud 
been  brought  to  the  hospital  as  accident  victims  even  though  their  injuries 
were  caused  by  assault.  A  few  innocent  patents  of  children  sLiffcting 
authentic  accidents  had  unfortunately  been  suspected  and  sometimes 
accused  of  abuse.  The  masquerading  of  abuse  as  an  accident  itml  the 
reverse  was  possible  because  neither  phenomenon  was  clearly  understood. 
The  investigators  decided  to  study  infant  accidents,  including  abmii,  to  liy 
to  pinpoint  the  characteristics  distinguishing  one  from  the  other. 

Subjects  were  infants  under  13  months  of  age  who  had  been  brought  lo 
Children's  Hospital  foi  x-ray  following  an  impact  accident  or  abusive 
incident.  Since  the  younger  the  baby  the  more  important  the  role  ol'tla1 
caretaker,  it  was  felt  that  this  study  would  yield  information  about 
parental  maltreatment  and  neglect,  and  not  be  complicated  by  (he 
considerations  of  the  noimal  accidents  of  the  active  toddler.  One  humhetl 
and  one  children  were  seen,  both  inpatients  and  outpatients.  Viirimis 
issues  were  explored:  for  instance,  the  difference  between  families  of 
abused  or  neglected  children  and  those  suffering  accidents,  will)  or 
without  injury.  The  differences  between  retarded  children  who  had  been 
abused  and  retarded  children  who  had  not  were  also  studied,  with  spccfol 
attention  to  the  mothering  received  by  each  group. 

Abuse  was  suspected  if  the  families1  explanation  of  the  injuries  weic 
not  adequate,  or  if  abuse  was  reported,  or  if  more  than  one  injury  WHS 
present.  Initial  assessments  were  made  of  family  stress.  Pregnancy  und 
very  small  children  were  considered  stressful.  The  family  was  conskleml 
to  be  under  strain,  too,  if  either  parent  had  a  close  relationship  wilh 
another  adult  who  was  unrelated  to  the  child.  This  issue  was  consider! 
and  observed  because  of  a  number  of  mass  media  reports  of  alniw 
involving  step-parents  of  pa.amours.  As  in  the  first  study,  this  did  not  turn 
out  to  be  an  important  issue. 

The  family  was  also  considered  under  stress  if  the  baby  had 
developmental  problems  such  as  a  significant  deviation  in  growth, 

m0t°r  °r  S0dal  devel°Pment>  or  such  troubles  as  feeding 
or  excessive  crying, 


r?  important  as  Parental  reports   are   heavily 
by  anxiety,  guilt,  and  concern  when  a  child  is  brought  to  a 

h  1  atrthe,entire  fam^  structure  to  find  the  clues  dial 
t,  n  T%famiiy:  When  abuse  is  ««P<*ted,  deficiencies  and 

S  v  bf  f?nd  ^  °ther  3Spects  of  fc"«y  ^.  This  study 
oK^"'  intfpersonal  relationships,  and  child  care 
order  to  illuminate  these  differentiating  issues.  Because  the 


352 


children  were  so  young,  the  mother  was  considered  the  principal  caretaker 
and  her  interaction  with  the  baby  was  carefully  examined.  In  order  to 
assess  the  mother-baby  relationship,  the  pair  were  observed  —  and  also  the 
mother  was  interviewed  and  asked  to  fill  out  questionnaires.  The 
observations  were  felt  to  be  of  particular  importance,  as  the  mother's 
habitual  behavior  with  the  child  is  likely  to  be  beyond  her  awareness  and 
ability  to  leport.  The  observations  provided  data  to  supplement  and 
correct  the  information  gained  through  the  other  methods. 

Many  issues  involved  with  mothering  weie  examined  in  addition  to  the 
traditional  ones  of  providing  food,  shelter,  and  medical  attention.  The 
stimulation  given  the  baby,  the  verbal  lesponsiveness,  the  quality  of  play, 
and  the  ability  of  the  mother  to  assess  the  changing  needs  of  the  child 
were  all  considered.  As  in  the  previous  study,  a  high  number  of  retarded 
cliildren  in  tlu's  group  was  noted  and  some  important  observations  about 
their  motheis  were  made. 

Attempts  to  have  an  equal  number  of  boys  and  girls,  white  and  non- 
white,  were  unsuccessful  as  the  potential  subjects  did  not  fall  this  way. 
Most  of  the  children  presented  for  x-iay  were  white  females.  Black  female 
babies  rarely  appeared  and,  in  addition,  it  was  inoie  difficult  to  enlist  the 
cooperation  of  the  nonwhite  families  in  the  research.  All  social  classes 
were  represented,  but  the  majority  of  the  families  were  in  the  lower 
classes. 

Although  a  few  families  had  refused  to  participate  in  the  first  study,  the 
refusal  rate  was  even  higher  in  the  second.  It  was  suspected  that  the 
increased  public  awareness  of  child  abuse  and  the  outcry  and  pressure  that 
had  been  building  up  made  some  parents  less  cooperative.  Also,  even 
parents  of  accidentally  injured  babies  experience  great  guilt  leading  to 
unwillingness  to  discuss  the  event.  In  the  Infant  Accident  Study,  it  was 
found  very  difficult  to  keep  the  allegedly  abusive  parents  as  subjects.  They 
rarely  refused  outright  to  come  to  an  appointment  or  to  allow  a  home 
visit,  but  they  failed  to  appear  or  were  away  from  home  at  the  specified 
time. 

Neglect  played  an  important  role,  and  several  families  who  could  not  be 
considered  abusive  were  still  considered  deficient  in  their  care  of  their 
children  based  on  observations  made  during  the  study.  Some  parents,  for 
instance,  left  their  children  without  competent  babysitters  when  they 
were  absent  for  prolonged  periods  of  time,  or  failed  to  obtain  needed 
medical  care  despite  repeated  and  careful  instructions  as  to  the  needs  of 
the  child. 

The  final  study  group  consisted  of  100  cases,  78  of  which  were 
followed  through  all  phases  of  the  study  and  22  in  which  families 
participated  in  the  initial  and  final  procedures,  with  only  a  mailed 
questionnaire  in  the  interim.  One  of  the  mothers  had  two  children  in  the 
study,  making  the  total  number  of  children  included  1 01 . 

The  methods  included  initial  screeening  of  x-rays,  several  home 
interviews  with  the  mothers,  and  observations  of  the  mothers  in 
examination,  feeding,  and  teaching  situations.  A  questionnaire  was  mailed, 
and  several  pediatric  and  developmental  evaluations  were  made.  In  several 

353 


situations  the  mother  was  put  under  mild  stress.  In  one  instance,  where 
she  was  asked  to  teach  her  baby  to  stack  a  series  of  blocks,  the  task  was 
generally  too  advanced  for  the  child  so  as  to  determine  her  reaction  when 
frustrated  by  the  baby.  In  another,  she  was  asked  to  fill  out  a 
questionnaire  when  accompanied  by  the  baby,  to  learn  about  her  attitude 
when  she  was  intent  on  another  task.  Four  of  the  research  persons  saw 
each  baby  and  family,  and  they  were  seen  in  as  many  situations  as  was 
practical. 

The  babies  were  evaluated  twice  in  the  first  two  years  of  life,  a  time  of 
extremely  rapid  growth  and  development.  This  allowed  for  observations 
of  the  effect  of  the  environment  in  a  way  not  possible  at  later  stages  of 
life.  Effects  of  poor  parental  care  are  obvious  very  soon  during  these  early 
stages. 

Of  the  101  babies,  brought  to  the  hospital,  only  10  were  entirely 
without  signs  or  symptoms.  These  1 0  had  been  brought  to  the  hospital  for 
examination  and  reassurance,  that  despite  a  potentially  injurious  event,  no 
injury  had  occurred.  The  other  91  babies  displayed  a  range  of  conditions 
from  mild  bruising  to  symptoms  related  to  the  central  nervous  system, 
such  as  momentary  unconsciousness  with  or  without  vomitting,  seizures, 
paralysis,  and  coma.  The  proportion  of  abused  children  without 
symptoms  was  roughly  equal  to  the  proportion  of  nonabused. 

In  addition  to  x-ray  examinations  of  the  site  of  the  presumed  injury,  21 
x-ray  surveys  of  the  entire  skeleton  were  performed.  Ten  of  these  were 
part  of  a  diagnostic  work-up  for  failure  to  thrive,  the  rest  because  multiple 
injuries  were  suspected.  Eighty-two  children  had  no  evidence  of  fracture, 
12  had  a  single  recent  fracture,  and  7  had  multiple  fractures.  The 
proportion  of  children  with  multiple  fractures  was  much  greater  among 
the  abused  than  the  nonabused  children.  It  was  thought  that  if  skeletal 
surveys  had  been  universally  performed,  they  might  have  disclosed  other 
unsuspected,  clinically  unimportant  fractures,  which  would  have  helped  to 
evaluate  the  quality  of  child  care.  However,  it  is  difficult  to  justify  x-ray 
examinations  without  symptoms  of  injury. 

Twenty-four  children  were  judged  to  be  abused,  or  to  be  both  abused 
and  neglected.  Ten  children  were  thought  to  be  neglected  only.  There 
were  67  nonabused,  non-neglected  children.  All  initial  judgments 
concerning  abuse  and  nonabuse  were  reevaluated  at  the  end  of  the  one- 
year  study. 

The  research  was  focused  on  the  effect  of  abuse  on  the  growing  infant, 
The  main  areas  of  investigation  included  mental  and  motor  development, 
behavioral  characteristics,  health  status,  and  physical  growth.  The  baby 
was  seen  as  being  affected  by  at  least  two  kinds  of  factors  -  those  that  are 
relatively  unchangeable,  such  as  conditions  at  birth,  and  those  that  are 
influenced  by  the  caretaker.  It  was  hypothesized  that  the  abusive  group, 
in  comparisoai  to  the  nonabuse  group,  would  show  more  stress,  less 
support,  and  greater  authoritarianism. 

As  required  by  State  law,  when  abuse  was  found,  reports  were  made  to 
the  Child  Welfare  Services  and  the  parents  were  informed.  Reports  were 
made  on  eight  children.  A  few  other  families  were  already  known  to  the 

354 


Child  Welfare  Services  when  they  came  into  the  study.  In  several  cases,  the 
mothers  had  named  their  husbands  as  the  abuser  and  had  separated  from 
them.  Two  mothers  overtly  rejected  the  children  whom  they  had 
mistreated,  and  the  study  personnel  helped  them  arrange  for  placement 
away  from  home.  The  protective  agency  removed  eight  children  from 
their  own  homes. 

Because  it  is  often  observed  that  sick  children  are  iiritable  and  difficult 
to  care  for,  it  was  noted  whether  or  not  the  child  had  an  acute  illness  at 
the  time  of  admission.  Eighteeen  of  the  babies  were  sick  with 
gastrointestinal  and  upper  respiratory  complaints  when  brought  to  the 
hospital.  A  few  had  anemia,  and  suffered  other  problems  such  as  eye 
infections.  By  and  large  the  babies  were  not  suffering  from  infections,  and 
the  traumatic  event  was  not  related  to  the  extra  demands  and  needs  of  a 
sick  child. 

Twenty-four  of  the  babies  were  admitted  to  the  hospital,  eleven  of 
these  for  protection  while  further  investigation  of  the  family  condition 
was  carried  out.  Thirteen  needed  hospital  medical-surgical  care,  some  for 
incidental  medical  problems  and  some  for  injuries  resulting  from  the 
accident  or  abuse. 

The  "failure  to  thrive"  babies  were  studied  from  several  standpoints. 
For  some,  metabolic  and  endocrine  studies  were  clone  with  inconclusive 
results.  This  condition,  defined  as  occurring  when  a  child  has  weight  and 
height  below  the  third  percentile  for  his  age  and  sex,  is  not  well 
understood.  Rarely  are  these  children  seen  because  of  trauma; 
characteristically,  the  mother  who  brings  them  in  is  full  of  concern 
because  a  child  has  not  reached  the  expected  developmental  landmarks. 
Often  she  is  anxious  because  her  child  is  not  growing  and  will  assert 
strongly  that  she  feeds  her  baby  well. 

Home  interviews  in  this  study  did  substantiate  that  some  of  these 
mothers  fed  their  babies  adequately.  Medical  opinion  is  growing  that  this 
entity  belongs  with  others  where  psychological  phenomena  and  physical 
development  interact  pathologically,  as  in  anorexia  nervosa  or  infantile 
marasmus.  Studies  at  Johns  Hopkins  Hospital  have  indicated  that  the 
problem  lay  in  the  hypothalmic  area  and  that  it  was  reversible  without 
any  hormonal  or  chemical  treatment  when  the  child  was  placed  in  a 
hospital,  a  relatively  nurturing  environment.  It  was  postulated  that 
emotional  disturbance  in  these  children  may  have  had  an  adverse  effect 
upon  the  release  of  the  pituitary  tropic  hormone  via  the  central  nervous 
system.  In  the  Infant  Accident  Study,  "failure  to  thrive"  babies  whose 
environment  was  changed  tended  to  achieve  normal  growth, but  rarely 
normal  development. 


Initial  Pediatnc  Evaluation 

Upon  initial  evaluation,  54  -  or  slightly  over  half  the  children  —  were 
found  to  have  either  no  medical  problems  or  only  the  insignificant  ones 
expected  during  the  first  year  of  life.  Two-thirds  of  the  abused  children, 

355 


benous  nealth  pioblems.  Slightly  fewer  than  half  the  babies 
had  a  number  of  actual  and/or  potential  health  problems  including 
prematurity,  moderate  or  high  peiinatal  stress  scores,  significant  medical 
problems,  and  histories  of  acute  illness,  Children  in  the  abused  group  had 
a  disproportionately  greater  number  of  health  problems  per  child  than  the 
nonabused  children. 

The  abused  group  was  also  distinguished  initially  from  the  nonabuseil 
by  their  poor  physical  growth.  In  part  their  retarded  growth  was  probably 
due  to  prematurity  (9  or  37  percent  were  premature  by  birth  weight, 
gestation*  or  both),  but  even  giving  credit  for  weeks  of  prematurity  did 
not  bring  them  to  normal  level. 

An  estimate  of  how  we'll  the  child  was  cared  for  in  general  was  judged 
by  the  manner  in  which  baby  appointments  and  immunizations  were 
attended  to  by  the  caretaker.  Nineteen  had  not  been  seen  regularly,  if  at 
all.  Twenty-eight  of  the  mothers  had  not  kept  their  babies1  immunization 
schedule  up-to-date,  and  some  babies  had  not  received  any  immunizations 
at  all.  Most  of  the  faulty  child  care  was  concentrated  in  the  abused  group. 
However,  upon  questioning  it  was  found  that  92  percent  of  all  tlie 
mothers  were  able  to  recognize  symptoms  of  poor  health  in  their  children 
and  to  find  suitable  medical  services  for  them  when  they  became  ill. 

Thirteen  babies  were  consideied  poorly  dressed,  dirty,  or  ill-kempt 
when  they  were  brought  for  their  pediatric  visit.  The  number  of  abused 
children  in  this  category  was  much  greater  than  the  number  of  nonabused. 


Accidents  Versus  Abuse  -  Initial  Findings 

Eighty-eight  caretakers  gave  an  accident  history.  Twelve  abused  children 
were  among  those  with  credible  accident  histories,  an  overlap  that 
illustrates  the  complexity  of  diagnosis  in  these  cases.  Thirteen  other 
children  either  had  totally  unexplained  injuries  or  they  were  x-raycd 
because  of  suspected  abuse,  but  no  injuries  were  found;  none  of  these  had 
an  accident  history. 

The  assumption  was  made  that  adequate  protection  by  the  caretakers 
could  completely  abolish  true  accidents.  The  investigators  realized, 
however,  that  this  is  unlikely  and  even  undesirable,  as  a  child  i  eared  in 
such  a  protective  environment  might  have  many  other  problems. 

Three-fifths  of  the  accidents  were  termed  "active"  because  the  baby's 
motor  activity  was  an  important  contributing  factor.  Active  accidents 
were  subdivided  into  three  categories:  "open  field"  in  which  the  babies 
propelled  themselves  into  danger  -  for  example,  falling  down  the  stairs 
when  a  gate  had  been  left  open;  falls  from  appropriate  furniture,  such  as 
couches  or  dressing  tables;  and  falls  from  inappropriate  furniture,  such  as 
the  tops  of  washing  machines  -  caused  for  instance,  when  a  baby  in  an 
infant  seat  wiggles  and  the  seat  slides  off  the  slippery  top  of  the  washing 
machine,  It  was  thought  that  most  of  the  active  accidents  might  have  been 
prevented  by  the  use  of  built-in  safety  devices,  such  as  belts  to  confine 
babies  on  dressing  tables. 

356 


Passive  accidents  were  those  in  which  the  baby's  contribution  was 
minor  and  the  responsibility  of  the  caretaker  greater.  Subdivisions  of 
passive  accidents  included  babies  dropped  by  their  caretakers,  those 
suffering  "Act  of  God"  events,  such  as  being  hit  by  a  stray  baseball,  and 
those  who  were  admittedly  assaulted  by  another  person. 

The  accidents  were  described,  then  the  abused  gioup  was  compared  to 
the  nonabused,  non-neglected  children  who  had  suffered  accidents.  Points 
of  comparison,  in  addition  to  general  health  and  injuries  already 
mentioned,  were  behavioral  characteristics,  age,  and  ordinal  position.  The 
families  were  compared  as  to  social  class,  stress  at  the  time  of  the  incident, 
and  health  of  the  mo theis. 

The  only  infants  who  differed  behaviorally  were  the  babies  who  had 
active,  open-field  accidents.  They  were  piedominantly  negative  in  mood, 
not  distractible,  and  moderately  or  highly  active.  This  combination  of 
traits  can  be  seen  to  result  in  babies  who  are  difficult  to  protect  from 
harm.  By  contrast,  the  other  subjects,  including  the  abused  children,  were 
positive  in  mood,  easily  distracted,  and  moderately  active.  The  babies 
represented  in  the  "open  field"  accidents  were  also  the  oldest  (median 
age,  42  weeks)  and,  therefore,  their  motor  development  was  more 
advanced. 

Most  of  the  babies  in  the  accident  group,  active  or  passive,  were  only 
children  in  their  families,  while  on  the  average  the  abused  child  was  the 
second  child.  This  suggest  that  parents  of  a  first  child  are  not  as  aware  of 
potential  hazards  as  they  might  be,  and  also  indicates  that  in  abuse  cases  - 
in  addition  to  evaluation  of  the  stress  of  having  several  small  children  -  the 
possibility  should  be  considered  that  one  small  child  might  injure  another. 

Ninety-two  percent  of  the  abusive  parents  were  identified  with  Class  V 
(low)  according  to  the  Hollingshead  Two-Factor  Index.  Forty-eight 
percent  of  the  nonabusive,  nonneglectful  families  studied  fell  in  this  class. 

Regardless  of  the  type  of  injurious  event,  the  mothers  typically  had 
special  stress  added  to  chronic  factors  of  strain.  The  abusive  mothers 
mentioned  baby  irritability  generally  and  the  other  more  often  mentioned 
disrupted  schedules,  fatigue,  etc.  Over  50  percent  of  the  mothers  of 
abused  children  had  significant  health  problems,  for  example,  mental 
retardation,  emotional  difficulties,  seizures,  and  heart  disease.  Such  major 
health  conditions  were  found  in  only  20  percent  of  the  nonabusive 
mothers. 

In  attempting  to  determine  the  quality  of  the  mother-child  relationship, 
the  investigators  studied  the  caretaking  process.  This  process  makes 
manifest  much  about  the  mother's  interaction  with  the  child  and  gives 
some  measure  of  her  general  ability  to  function.  The  ability  of  the  mother 
to  monitor  the  environment  for  her  helpless  baby  changes  in  relationship 
to  many  things.  The  mother,  of  course,  operates  within  her  own  milieu 
and  class  structure,  and  this  partially  defines  good  mothering  for  her.  The 
events  that  occur,  the  health  and  stability  of  the  mother,  the  abilities  she 
is  potentially  able  to  bring  to  bear  to  help  her  child,  and  her  ability  to 
perceive  accurately  his  needs  all  affect  her  care  of  the  child.  Her  degree  of 
affection  as  well  as  her  convictions  about  child  rearing  also  enter  in. 

357 


undoubtedly,  too,  the  resources  of  the  mother  to   provide  support, 
affection,  and  aid  for  her  are  crucial. 

Stress  is  seen  as  a  major  issue  in  child  abuse,  the  caretaker  being  under 
insupportable  stiessin  most  such  situations.  In  the  case  of  the  accidents,  it 
has  been  noted  that  most  of  the  mothers  were  reacting  to  stress  in  varying 
degrees  —  the  abusive  mothers,  however,  were  under  greater  stress,  had  less 
support,  and  fewer  personal  resources.  The  interrelationship  of  stress, 
support  and  the  ability  to  cope  can  be  seen  as  a  continuum. 

It  must  be  remembered  that  the  early  years  of  child  rearing  are  heavily 
demanding.  Little  money  is  available,  and  many  young  people  aic 
unprepared  to  become  parents.  Shifts  occm  in  families  even  when  children 
are  desired  and  planned  for,  and  greater  strain  is  felt  with  unwanted 
babies.  The  investigators  view  most  young  families  as  being  at  a  point  of 
lowest  toleiance  for  stress  at  a  time  when  they  are  subjected  to  the  highest 
stress  during  the  years  when  childien  are  being  born.  However,  it  must  be 
realized  that  what  constitutes  stress  for  one  individual  can  be  handled  by 
another.  Unfortunately,  some  types  of  chronic  stress  are  brought  on  by 
poverty,  which  rarely  permits  growth  or  learning  and  usually 
undermines  a  family's  ability  to  function.  The  investigators  were  most 
interested  in  everyday  stress,  as  opposed  to  extraordinary  or  emergency 
stress,  because  they  felt  that  chronic  stress  was  of  key  importance  in  child 
abuse. 

The  demographic  data  about  the  abusive  families  was  a  documentation 
of  the  degree  of  chronic  stress.  Almost  half  the  abused  children  were 
black,  and  most  of  the  combined  abuse  and  neglect  occurred  among  the 
black  families.  Because  of  the  larger  numbers  of  children  with 
comparatively  few  fathers  in  these  homes,  the  families  fall  into  a  gioup 
known  to  be  especially  vulnerable  to  many  kinds  of  stress.  According  to 
socioeconomic  status,  all  the  families  in  the  combined  abuse  and  neglect 
group  are  classified  in  the  two  lowest  classes  of  the  Hollingshead  scale  of 
social  position. 

There  were  ten  families  that  were  considered  neglectful  but  not  abusive, 
and  it  was  found  that  in  some  ways  they  resembled  both  the  abusive  and 
nonabusive  families. 

The  nine  families  demonstrating  abuse  but  not  neglect  were 
predominantly  lower  class,  with  two  members  of  the  middle  and  upper 
classes;  the  ten  families  showing  neglect  alone  included  four  classes  from 
Class  I,  the  highest  of  Hollingshead 's  classifications.  One  of  these  families 
was  classified  as  neglectful  because  they  habitually  left  the  baby  in  the 
company  of  an  active  30-month  old  sibling  without  adult  supervision, 
Another  family  was  called  neglectful  because  their  child  was  encouraged 
to  perform  physical  feats  distinctly  beyond  his  limited  ability,  such  as 
hanging  from  the  pantry  shelf  by  his  fingers. 

An  assessment  was  made  of  the  medical  condition  of  the  child  and  tlie 
mother's  reaction  to  it.  It  was  found  that  of  the  ten  "neglect  only" 
mothers,  eight  showed  only  slight  reactions  to  medical  problems.  The 
mothers  of  the  abused  children  reported  feeling  great  stress  due  to  their 
babies1  medical  conditions.  The  focus  of  these  mothers  on  their  babies' 

358 


health  was  seen  by  the  investigators  as  being  realistic  in  view  of  the  ex- 
tremely poor  health  of  these  children. 

There  was  one  group  of  24  women,  primarily  made  up  of  black 
mothers,  who  were  unusually  bland  or  under-reacting  to  medical  problems 
in  both  themselves  and  their  children.  One  such  mother  had  noted  an 
abnormality  in  her  baby's  eye  for  more  than  a  week,  but  hadn't  sought 
medical  advice  about  it.  The  investigators  had  several  ideas,  but  no 
definite  answers  to  explain  this  attitude.  A  middle-class  mother,  they 
believe,  tends  to  emphasize  her  attentiveness  to  her  child  whether  or  not  it 
is  warranted.  This  is  not  always  the  case  with  the  lower  socioeconomic 
class  mother.  The  investigators  also  note  that  many  of  the  "upper 
reactors"  were  poorly  educated  and  suggest  that  perhaps  they  did  not 
really  understand  the  potential  hazard  of  some  of  the  conditions. 
However,  seven  of  the  "under-reactors"  belonged  to  middle-  and  upper- 
class  groups  and  were  well  educated.  An  analysis  of  the  "under  reactors" 
by  social  class  showed  no  significant  class  association.  It  is  also  suggested 
that  the  appaient  apathy  may  be  a  defense  against  implied  criticism  and 
intrusion  by  the  outsider  or  a  way  of  coping  with  what  would  otherwise 
be  overwhelming  anxiety.  It  may  also  indicate  a  true  indifference  or  a 
general  state  of  apathy  which  includes,  but  is  not  limited  to,  the  child. 

The  type  of  stimulus  perceived  as  stressful  and  the  reaction  to  it  are 
highly  individualized  matters.  To  avoid  imposing  any  preconceived 
hierarchy  of  stress,  the  investigators  inquired  what  had  happened  to  the 
mothers  or  their  families  since  conception  for  the  index  child.  The  events 
divided  naturally  into  Hill's  four  categories:  physical  difficulties, 
separations  from  persons  or  possessions,  accession  events  such  as  a  new 
person  moving  into  the  house,  and  social  disgrace.  The  mothers'  reactions 
were  dichotomized  as  1)  mild  or  non-existent,  or  2)  strong.  The  number 
of  stress  events  and  associated  reactions  were  combined  to  yield  a  total 
score  for  each  individual.  Mothers  in  each  group  were  ranked  and  the 
groups  compaied. 

In  general,  recent  events  were  given  highest  stress  ratings  by  the 
mothers.  Acute  conditions  were  reported  as  more  stressful  than  chronic 
ones,  and  events  involving  the  immediate  family  as  more  stressful  than 
those  involving  extended  family  or  friends.  The  proportion  of  mothers 
reporting  no  stress  whatsoever  was  greater  among  the  neglectful  than  the 
nondeviant,  and  only  4  percent  of  the  abusive  reported  no  stress. 

Accidents,  moves,  and  physical  illness  were  the  greatest  sources  of  stress 
for  the  abusive  mothers;  also,  prominent  among  the  accidents  were  attacks 
upon  the  mothers  by  others.  Several  women  reported  having  been  beaten 
by  their  husbands.  One  abusive  mother  claimed  to  have  been  raped  on  her 
way  to  the  hospital  with  her  baby.  Although  the  report  seemed 
questionable,  it  was  similar  to  others  in  its  preoccupation  with  violence, 
either  factual  or  fantasized. 

A  common  source  of  stress  for  the  abusive  and  other  families  was  a 
change  of  residence.  Typically  the  family  moved  during  the  woman's 
pregnancy  to  obtain  more  room.  In  both  the  abusive  and  nondeviant 

359 


groups,  tlie  moves  in  late  months  of  pregnancy  resulted  in  strain  on  tJic 
mothers  and  at  times  brought  on  premature  deliveries. 

Illness  was  often  reported  and  sometimes  -  especially  in  the  group  of 
abusive  mothers  -  reported  not  as  chronic  conditions,  but  related  to 
pregnancy  for  the  index  child.  The  abusive  mothers  felt  very  strained  by 
the  pregnancy  of  the  child  in  question  and  ranked  pregnancy  as  a  higher 
stress  even  than  deaths. 

According  to  the  physician's  rankings  of  physical  disorders  associated 
with  pregnancy,  the  abusive  mothers  who  reported  the  most  sticss 
actually  had  the  least,  thus  indicating  a  higher  psychological  sensitivity. 
Six  of  the  abusive  mothers  who  reported  difficult  pregnancies  weic  caring 
for  other  young  babies  when  they  were  pregnant  and,  also,  had  fewer 
people  available  to  help  them. 

Potential  support  factors  included  a  satisfactory  male  relationship:  the 
presence  of  a  man  in  the  house  —  whether  spouse,  common-law  partner,  or 
father;  a  continuous  association  with  a  male  during  pregnancy;  help  fiom 
the  man  in  relation  to  the  baby;  a  stable  source  of  income;  a  continuous 
source  of  medical  care;  participation  in  religious  activities  and  involvement 
with  neighborhood  activities.  The  abusive  families  and  the  others  differed 
significantly  in  the  amount  of  support  available.  The  abusive  families  had 
the  least  support. 

Statistically  significant  factors  were  continuous  association  with  (lie 
father  during  pregnancy  and  help  from  a  male  —  whether  husband,  father, 
or  friend  -  in  relation  to  the  baby.  The  current  presence  of  a  male  in  (he 
home  on  a  stable  basis  did  not  appear  to  be  a  significant  positive  factor; 
nor  was  marital  stress  a  significant  negative  factor.  During  pregnancy  (he 
help  of  the  father  appeared  to  be  mainly  psychological,  but  once  the  baby 
arrived  the  mother  received  more  support  when  some  male  did  something 
concrete  to  assist  in  the  care  of  the  baby. 

The  effects  of  race  on  stress  and  support  were  assessed.  When  support 
was  low,  black  mothers  reported  significantly  more  medical  stress  Mum 
white  mothers;  they  suffered  more  physical  problems  than  the  whites.  The 
white  mothers,  however,  reported  significantly  more  social  stress  when 
support  was  low.  This  comparison  would  seem  to  reflect  the  perception  of 
the  woman,  white  or  black,  as  she  viewed  herself  in  relation  to  her  peer 
group. 

The  mothers  were  scored  for  general  negative  and  positive  reactions  to 
their  babies,  and  several  trends  appeared.  The  abusive  mothers  tended 
toward  extreme  reactions,  judging  the  babies  to  be  either  all  good  or  all 
bad,  while  the  non-abusive  parents  saw  their  children  more  realistically  as 
both  pleasing  and  annoying.  The  abusive  mothers  were  relatively  silent 
with  regard  to  their  children's  development.  Fifteen  of  the  19  abused 
children  showed  early  signs  of  retardation, 

360 


Modes  of  Punishment,  Discipline,  and  Teaching 

It  was  predicted  that  the  abusive  mothers  would  use  harsher  methods  of 
punishment  and  would  have  less  interest  in  teaching  their  children  than 
the  other  mothers.  The  results  were  not  so  clear  cut. 

Forty-one  percent  of  all  the  mothers  used  some  form  of  physical 
punishment  with  babies  less  than  6  months  old,  usually  slapping  the  hands 
or  the  buttocks.  At  9  months,  physical  punishment  intensified.  By  24 
months,  87  percent  of  all  the  mothers  were  using  this  method  of  physical 
punishment  at  least  part  of  the  time. 

The  type  of  behavior  punished  varied  with  social  class.  Mothers  in  the 
two  highest  classes  punished  principally  for  aggressive  acts;  middle-class 
mothers  for  activity,  dangerous  or  otherwise;  and  lower-class  mothers,  for 
conduct  such  as  excessive  demands,  disobedience,  or  crying.  Generally  the 
abusive  motheis,  most  of  whom  were  lower  class,  punished  for 
unacceptable  conduct.  Across  all  groups,  girls  were  consistently  punished 
earlier:  by  the  age  of  9  months,  3 1  percent  of  the  girls,  but  only  5  percent 
of  the  boys  were  being  punished;  by  18  months,  the  figures  rose  to  70 
percent  and  50  percent  for  the  girls  and  boys,  respectively. 

The  investigators  had  become  aware  that  most  mothers  are  extremely 
sensitive  to  their  babies'  aggressive  acts  against  them  as  mothers.  When 
asked  how  they  would  respond  if  their  infants  struck  them  or  spat  upon 
them,  the  overwhelming  majority  of  the  mothers  of  babies  6  months  of 
age  or  older  said  they  would  retaliate  in  kind,  **.  .  .to  show  him  that  he  is 
not  to  do  that  kind  of  thing."  Three  mothers  of  babies  who  were  less 
than  6  mo  nths  of  age  also  said  the  same .  Eighty  percent  of  the  abusive  and 
63  percent  of  the  nondeviant  mothers  said  they  would  hit  back  against 
infant  aggression. 

Regardless  of  their  social  class,  most  mothers  asserted  that  a  baby 
should  know  right  from  wrong  by  the  age  of  12  months,  and  one-third  of 
the  mothers  specified  6  months.  This  belief  implies  a  common  lack  of 
realistic  information  about  infant  development  and  when  babies  learn 
concepts  of  right  and  wrong.  These  mothers  also  perceived  the  babies  as 
having  "tempers"  and  other  directed  feelings  at  a  much  younger  age  than 
is  actually  possible. 

The  mothers  involved  in  this  study  usually  discriminated  very  little 
between  discipline  and  teaching.  When  asked  how  they  would  attempt  to 
teach  the  baby  some  new  behavior  representing  a  real  learning  effort  for 
him,  they  most  frequently  responded  in  terms  of  scolding  or  spanking  to 
get  him  to  learn  after  first  giving  verbal  instructions.  The  investigators  feel 
that  infants  are  punished  physically  more  often  than  is  realized.  When  it  is 
common  practice  to  strike  babies,  however  lightly,  with  the  goal  of 
teaching  them,  the  laws  of  probability  indicate  that  some  babies  are  going 
to  be  struck  too  hard  and  that  some  will  be  injured. 

361 


neiaiea  to  Mothering 

The  mothers  were  questioned  as  to  their  expectations  concerning  the 
child,  The  majority  preferred  their  babies  to  be  "good";  that  is,  respectful, 
grateful,  obedient,  and  not  rebellious.  These  were  the  particular  goals  of 
the  abusive  and  neglectful  mothers;  they  were  not  interested  in  creativity, 
etc. 

There  were  varied  opinions  among  them  as  to  what  constituted  an 
"ideal  mother."  All  the  abusive  and  the  neglectful  mothers  mentioned 
keeping  the  baby  clean  and  giving  him  material  things.  A  few  mentioned 
the  importance  of  being  a  "proper"  woman.  They  described  the  ideal 
woman  in  negative  terms  as  somebody  who  does  not  run  around  or  sit  at 
bars.  The  abusive  group  often  described  the  ideal  father  in  terms  of 
discipline  or  financial  support.  Several  women  said  that  the  ideal  father 
should  not  beat  the  mother. 

Quite  a  few  mothers  felt  that  affection  should  be  restrained:  thai  (hero 
is  danger  in  being  too  affectionate  toward  babies.  This  trait  was  more 
marked  in  the  abusive  and  the  neglectful  mothers. 

The  index  of  values  related  to  mothering  clearly  and  significantly 
distinguished  between  the  abusive  and  the  nonabusive  groups,  correctly 
classifying  77  percent  of  all  the  families.  Among  the  abusive  mothers, 
emphasis  was  placed  on  cleanliness  and  materialistic  values.  They  tended 
to  perceive  themselves  and  their  husbands  in  stereotyped  roles,  n 
perception  suggesting  difficulties  in  forming  and  maintaining  close 
relationships.  Their  fear  of  showing  too  much  affection  toward  llicir 
babies  was  another  manifestation  of  the  same  difficulty.  These 
characteristics,  together  with  the  common  need  to  have  an  obedient, 
compliant  baby,  established  the  abusive  mothers  as  more  authoritarian 
than  the  nonabusive  women. 


The  Baby 

The  contribution  of  the  baby  to  the  mother-child  relationship  is 
extremely  important.  A  smiling  baby  who  is  responsive  may  keep  even  a 
detached  mother  involved.  While  many  types  of  behavior  are  important,  it 
was  decided  that  four  behavioral  characteristics  would  be  examined: 
mood,  level  of  activity,  approach  or  the  way  the  baby  related  to  a  new 
person  or  new  object,  and  distractibility.  These  characteristics  were 
studied  during  the  initial  and  the  final  pediatric  examinations. 

Among  all  the  children  the  distribution  of  positive  and  negative  mood 
showed  a  decided  difference  according  to  sex  and  developmental  age, 
Regardless  of  their  developmental  age,  half  the  boys  were  positive,  half 
negative.  The  girls  were  strikingly  negative  when  developmentally  young 
but  became  positive  as  they  matured.  Abused  boys  were  markedly 
negative  compared  to  their  nonabused  peers.  Abused  girls  were  more 
positive  than  nonabused  girls;  however,  the  abused  females  as  a  group 
were  developmentally  older  than  the  nonabused. 

362 


Eight  abused  children,  four  boys  and  four  girls,  were  separated  from 
their  parents  by  the  time  of  the  second  testing.  All  were  predominantly 
negative  in  mood.  According  to  the  study  data,  this  seemed  associated  less 
with  their  removal  from  home  and  more  with  the  mood  to  be  expected 
from  the  above  findings.  The  four  negative  boys  were  similar  in  mood  to 
the  majority  of  abused  males,  while  three  of  the  negative  girls  were 
developmentally  young  and  thus  apt  to  be  more  negative. 

The  factors  of  sex,  age,  and  abuse  which  affect  predominant  mood  need 
considerably  more  study  before  the  interrelationships  will  become  clear. 
Nevertheless,  these  findings  suggest  that  boys  and  girls  may  indeed 
respond  to  abuse  in  quite  different  ways. 

It  was  not  possible  to  find  associations  between  mental  development 
and  behavioral  patterns.  Some  of  the  children  were  advanced 
developmentally,  some  at  age  level,  and  some  retarded.  Some  in  each 
group  were  positive  in  mood  and  could  be  distracted. 

Distractibility  depends  upon  whether  or  not  a  child  can  be  intrigued 
away  from  something  he  is  doing,  especially  if  it  is  a  hazardous  activity.  In 
this  case,  it  is  a  positive  quality,  as  opposed  to  hyperactive  distractibility 
which  interferes  with  concentrated  learning. 

All  of  the  babies  who  were  positive  in  mood  and  distractibility  were 
positive  in  approach;  that  is,  interested  or  curious  or  pleased  at  meeting 
new  people  and  new  things.  Among  the  nonabused  children  who  had 
negative  or  mixed  scores  on  mood  and  distractibility,  a  racial  difference 
occurred  on  the  approach  scores.  The  whites  of  both  sexes  were 
predominantly  positive  in  approach,  while  the  blacks  of  both  sexes  were 
predominantly  negative.  The  investigators  noted  that  the  examining 
doctor  was  a  white  woman  and  wondered  if  this  could  be  a  factor 
influencing  these  results. 

Activity  levels  did  not  distinguish  between  abused  and  nonabused 
children.  The  babies  who  were  both  abused  and  neglected  were  low  in 
approach  and  play  behavior  and  high  in  negative  activity.  The  "abused 
only"  or  "neglect  only"  had  wider  repertoires  of  behavior.  However,  of 
the  "neglected-only"  children,  a  large  proportion  either  remained  high  or 
became  high  in  activity.  It  was  observed  that  the  mothers  were  largely 
ineffectual  in  controlling  their  children;  they  tended  to  pile  on 
command-after-command  while  the  children  became  more  anxious,  active, 
and  difficult.  The  ability  of  these  mothers  to  limit  the  activity  of  their 
children,  who  seemed  to  be  in  special  need  of  help  in  controlling  or 
directing  their  activities,  seemed  very  meager. 


Observations  of  Interaction 

The  mothers  were  observed  with  their  babies  during  a  feeding  period  at 
home  and  in  a  teaching  situation  in  the  pediatrician's  office.  It  was  learned 
that  mothers  vary  considerably  in  their  perceptions  of  what  is  dangerous 
to  their  babies.  While  the  mothers  were  in  the  doctor's  office,  the  babies 
were  often  attracted  to  the  doctor's  kit  containing  instruments.  Most 

363 


mothers  did  not  permit  their  children  to  handle  these,  yet  failed  to  see  the 
danger  in  the  sharp  corner  of  a  drawer  that  the  children  loved  to  pull  out. 
The  examining  table  was  also  a  danger,  as  mothers  often  turned  away 
while  their  babies  who  were  lying  on  it  waited  to  be  dressed. 

The  actions  of  each  baby  and  mother  were  tallied  and  analyzed 
according  to  content,  mode,  and  context.  Mothers  of  retarded  children 
concentrated  on  feeding  them  and  behaved  more  positively  toward  the 
child  as  the  baby  ate.  They  did  not  talk  spontaneously  to  the  child  as 
much  as  the  mothers  of  non-retarded  children.  The  investigators  believed 
that  this  reflected  the  mother's  concern  that  the  child  eat  rather  than  play 
or  socialize.  There  was  an  overwhelming  tendency  for  mothers  of  retarded 
abused  children  to  show  low  verbal  response  to  the  babies'  vocalizations, 
but  an  opposite  trend  was  shown  by  mothers  of  retarded  nonabitsed 
children.  The  age  of  the  baby  was  not  a  factor  in  the  mother's  tendency  to 
verbalize  when  the  baby  made  sounds.  Some  babies  responded  to  tlicir 
mothers'  speech,  others  did  not,  and  again  this  was  not  related  to  b;iby 
age. 

It  is  probable  that  an  involved  mother  gives  her  baby  many  types  of 
stimulation  in  addition  to  the  verbal.  The  verbal  response,  however,  seems 
to  be  a  good  indicator  of  the  total  social  environment  provided  for  the 
baby.  Mothers  with  a  good  education  were  much  more  verbally  responsive 
than  those  with  a  poor  education, 

A  significant  association  appeared  between  mothers  who  responded 
verbally  and  the  higher  rates  of  development  among  these  babies,  The 
investigators  note  that  such  an  association  has  not  previously  been 
reported  but  they  point  to  several  conditions  that  may  affect  it.  The 
children  of  well-educated,  intelligent  mothers  may  have  superior  genetic 
endowment.  Also,  babies  who  have  had  a  great  deal  of  verbal  experience 
.  do  better  on  tests,  which  often  require  ability  to  follow  verbid 
instructions. 

With  regard  to  control,  the  abusive  mothers  tended  to  give  their 
children  great  latitude  until  their  patience  wore  thin,  when  they  would 
abruptly  threaten  or  strike  their  children.  The  neglectful  mother  seemed 
to  burden  the  child  with  repetitive  commands  and  threats  to  which  lie 
paid  little  attention,  apparently  sensing  that  the  mother  did  not  know 
how  to  control  him,  or  for  some  reason  was  unable  to  do  so.  As  lie 
became  more  active,  the  mother  became  more  frenetic. 

The  abusive  and  neglectful  mothers  tended  to  care  for  the  babies,  but 
made  neither  broader  responses  nor  extra  reactions  to  their  children.  They 
tended  toward  stereotyped  responses. 

The  teaching  situation,  which  was  essentially  an  artificial  one,  aroused 
some  anxiety.  However,  the  observers  of  the  feeding,  who  knew  the 
mothers,  thought  that  they  behaved  much  the  same  as  they  had  in  the 
past.  This  was  substantiated  by  the  significant  positive  correlations 
between  the  feeding  and  teaching  observations  with  respect  to 
maternal-verbal  responses  among  all  cases;  mothers  of  nonretarded 
children;  high  social  class;  and  females, 

364 


Final  EvaEuation  of  the  Babies 

The  most  important  final  difference  between  the  abused  and  the 
nonabused  babies  appeared  in  the  scores  on  the  Bayley  Mental  Scales. 
There  was  significantly  more  retardation  among  the  abused  children  when 
compared  with  the  nonabused.  The  likelihood  of  retarded  mental 
development  among  the  abused  children  was  greatly  increased  when  they 
were  also  judged  to  be  neglected  by  their  parents.  The  fairly  high  rate  of 
mental  retardation  found  in  all  groups  of  children  in  this  study  may  mean 
that  the  hospital  outpatient  population  is  biased  in  this  direction. 

The  final  checkups  showed  little  difference  in  height  and  weight 
increases;  between  the  abused  and  nonabused  children.  However,  this  was 
true  largely  because  one-third  of  the  abused  childien  had  been  removed 
from  their  homes  and  placed  in  benign  and  maturing  homes.  All  but  one 
showed  remarkable  catch-up  growth.  There  was  a  significant  association 
between  height  and  mental  development  ratings,  with  retarded  mental 
development  occurring  more  frequently  among  children  below  the  10th 
percentile  in  height. 

In  terms  of  family  characteristics,  Ike  single  factor  most  strongly 
related  to  the  mental  development  ratings  was  the  amount  of  income  per 
person  in  the  household.  The  percentage  of  children  within  the  retarded, 
normal,  and  advanced  groups  coming  from  families  with  less  than  $100  a 
month  per  person  was  74  percent,  46  percent,  and  7  percent  respectively. 
Although  abused  children  more  often  come  from  families  with  low 
incomes,  the  relationship  between  income  and  mental  development  was 
not  altered  significantly  when  controlled  for  the  occurrence  of  abuse. 
When  children  with  retarded  and  normal  development  were  combined,  79 
percent  of  the  abused  and  52  percent  of  the  nonabused  children  came 
from  low-income  families;  this  represents  a  statistically  significant 
difference. 

Among  those  with  advanced  development,  the  majority  of  the  children 
were  white;  among  those  with  slow  development,  the  majority  were  black. 

Although  stress  was  found  important,  no  statistical  association  between 
ratings  of  social  stress  and  ratings  of  mental  development  appeared. 
However,  the  number  of  supportive  resources  for  the  mother  was  related 
to  mental  development.  Among  the  families  of  retarded  abused  children, 
69  percent  were  low  in  support  while  only  31  percent  of  the  families  of 
nonabused  retarded  children  had  similar  low  ratings.  When  stress  hits  a 
family  with  few  sources  of  support  and  assistance,  then  the  problems 
become  more  intense. 

The  presence  of  the  father  is  also  important.  The  group  who  were 
advanced  mentally  all  had  their  fathers  at  home,  while  only  59  percent  of 
the  retarded  had  fathers  living  with  them.  The  father  was  absent  in  the 
cases  of  64  percent  of  the  retarded  children  who  were  abused. 

Regarding  the  probability  of  retardation,  three  factors  in  addition  to 
abuse  are  important:  low  monthly  income  per  person,  significant  physical 
problems  in  the  baby,  and  low  verbal  responsiveness  in  the  mother.  When 
any  two  of  these  factors  plus  abuse  was  present,  1 00  percent  of  the 

365 


cmiciren  weie  retarded.  Among  children  without  any  of  these  factors,  only 
21  percent  weie  retarded. 

The  second  evaluations  showed  that,  remarkably  enough,  there  are 
cliiidren  who  appear  normal  despite  abuse,  and  it  is  also  evident  that  there 
is  a  range  of  intensity  in  abuse.  Some  children  are  subjected  to  pervasive 
and  long-standing  abuse,  while  for  others  the  abusive  incident  is  isolated  in 
an  otherwise  favorable  environment.  The  investigators  caution  that  an 
overall  characterization  of  the  abused  child  demands  both  pediatric  and 
family  assessment  The  physical  and  mental  effects  of  abuse  can  be 
mimicked  by  other  conditions,  and  also  the  physical  and  mental  state  inav 
not  fully  expose  the  abusive  atmosphere  of  the  home. 


Diagnosis  of  Abuse 

A  crucial  factor  in  the  diagnosis  of  abuse  is  the  willingness  of  tlw 
physician  to  consider  abuse  as  a  possible  cause  of  a  child's  injuries  iincl  lo 
examine  him  accordingly.  Dr.  Grace  Gregg,  a  pediatrician  and  an 
investigator  in  this  study,  points  out  that  a  diagnosis  of  abuse  requires  .1 
history  that  fails  to  explain  the  injury,  the  elimination  of  systemic  disease, 
and  an  assessment  of  the  type  of  care  that  would  allow  such  a  condition 
to  develop.  "Failure  to  tlirive"  children  must  be  looked  at  with  an  eye  lo 
abuse  and  neglect.  It  is  important  where  there  are  multiple  injuries  thiit 
each  be  accounted  for.  Multiple  bone  injuries  are  considered  a  key 
indication  of  abusive  treatment.  It  must  be  remembered,  however,  Hint 
some  bone  changes  do  not  show  up  immediately  on  x-ray  and  may  be 
hidden  until  about  12  to  14  days.  Furthermore,  x-ray  can  tell  Hie 
condition  of  the  bones,  but  not  how  they  were  injured  nor  the  motivation 
of  the  person  responsible  for  the  injury.  In  some  cases  a  parent  can 
roughly  and  abruptly  grab  a  child  to  prevent  an  injury  and  accidentally 
hurt  him.  However,  while  this  type  of  accident  can  happen  once,  a  scries 
of  such  incidents  would  be  highly  suspicious.  Also,  the  idea  that  siblings 
can  injure  infants  is  unpopular,  but  must  be  considered. 

Malnutrition  is  a  key  indication  of  abuse  or  neglect,  but  evidence  of 
malnutrition  is  difficult  to  identify  when  intake  becomes  adequate,  unless 
photographs  are  taken. 

Familiarity  with  the  normal  injuries  of  children  is  indispensable  to 
adequate  diagnosis.  Superficial  injuries  above  the  elbows,  shins,  and  knees 
that  do  not  resemble  dermatologic  conditions  should  be  examined  to  sec 
if  they  have  been  caused  by  rough  handling,  human  bites,  cigarette  burns, 
etc.  All  bones  and  joints  should  be  examined,  not  merely  those  pointed 
out  as  injured. 


Legal  Issues 

By  June  1967,  52  child  abuse  reporting  laws  were  in  existence  in  all  50 
States,  the  Virgin  Islands,  and  the  District  of  Columbia.  Puerto  Rico 

366 


added  a  law  soon  thereafter.  In  most  cases  mandatory  reporting  by 
medical  personnel,  occasionally  by  schools  or  social  workers,  and 
investigation  by  law  enforcement  agencies  was  typical.  In  some  states  the 
professional  can  be  fined  or  imprisoned  if  he  fails  to  report  a  case  of  child 
abuse. 

At  first  glance  it  looks  as  though  the  situation  has  been  acknowledged 
and  adequately  coveied.  However,  this  is  far  from  the  case.  The  Children's 
Bureau  looks  upon  child  abuse  reporting  laws  as  case-finding  devices.  How 
successful  they  are  for  this  purpose  is  difficult  to  assess.  Some  problems 
have,  however,  been  identified.  For  instance,  diagnostic  guidelines  are  not 
well-drawn.  There  is  the  possibility  of  an  inappropriate  accusation, 
perhaps  a  law  suit.  Medical  training  is  often  limited  in  terms  of  teaching 
the  type  of  social-family  assessment  that  is  required  in  many  child  abuse 
cases.  This  type  of  case  can  be  tremendously  time-consuming  and  time  is  a 
rare  commodity  in  most  medical  practice. 

Another  problem  is  that  of  confidentiality.  The  child  abuse  laws  place 
the  child's  right  to  safety  above  the  traditional  rights  of  the  patient  —  in 
this  case,  the  parent  —  to  protected  communications,  A  social  class 
difference  may  slant  the  manner  in  which  a  case  is  treated.  For  example, 
in  many  States  hospitals  and  physicians  are  required  to  report  a  case  if  an 
abused  child  is  brought  for  care.  Private  doctors,  however,  are  not  exposed 
to  the  same  public  attention  and  might  -  and  do  -  manage  private  patients 
differently.  A  doctor  who  reports  a  patient  stands  to  gain  ill-will  and  lose 
the  family  for  treatment.  He  may  feel  he  can  give  more  help  by  not 
reporting  the  case  and  staying  involved  as  an  interested  and  concerned 
family  doctor.  Patients  who  can  afford  private  care  can  also  "shop" 
around.  They  can  go  to  different  physicians  and  the  full  extent  of  the 
child's  history  of  trauma  might  be  hidden  in  this  way. 

The  goal  is  not,  it  must  be  remembered,  merely  full  reporting.  The  goal 
is  the  protection  of  the  child.  The  two  are  related,  but  not  the  same. 

The  investigators  feel  that  professionals  who  report  a  family  in  good 
faith  should  be  granted  immunity  in  the  event  of  a  law  suit.  Other  changes 
should  allow  for  concern  for  the  other  children  in  the  family. 

The  lack  of  community  placements  for  such  endangered  children  also 
make  some  people  reluctant  to  confront  an  abusive  parent,  as  it  is  realized 
that  all  too  often  the  child  must  return  home  with  an  even  more  enraged 
and  abusive  parent. 

There  is  considerable  question  as  well,  as  to  whether  or  not  the  police 
should  be  given  responsibility  for  establishing  whether  abuse  has  occurred. 
In  many  cases  this  type  of  approach  with  the  goal  of  proving  guilt  and 
establishing  criminal  behavior  is  unfruitful.  As  this  research  has  shown,  the 
problem  may  be  subtle;  a  child  may  have  been  left  with  inadequate 
supervision  or  the  parent  may  have  shown  poor  ability  to  anticipate  the 
child's  pattern  of  activity.  The  caretakers  may  be  extremely  immature  or 
disturbed,  and  thus  cannot  be  considered  directly  responsible  for  injury  to 
the  child.  Nonetheless  the  child  may  be  in  great  jeopardy.  But  it  is 
questionable  whether  most  police  have  the  orientation,  time,  or  training 
to  investigate  these  issues. 

367 


Prevention  is  largely  ignoied,  as  is  appropriate  follow-up  and  assistance, 
to  families  in  need  of  community  aid.  Expanded  protective  services  ;uc 
much  to  be  desired.  Assistance  with  related  problems  such  as 
contraception  should  also  be  available. 

Punishment  of  the  parents  or  probation,  which  often  means  only  Ihu 
most  minimal  surveillance,  rarely  accomplishes  much  toward  the  most 
important  goal  -  protection  of  the  child.  Punishment  for  doctors  or 
hospitals  may  very  well  be  self-defeating  and  discouiagc  reporting.  The 
most  fruitful  approach  is  via  education.  Physicians  need  to  be  sens!  li/ccl  U) 
the  issues  and  hospital  procedures  need  to  be  changed  so  as  to  permit 
early  identification  of  the  endangered  child.  Refeirals  to  social  agencies 
should  be  facilitated  and  child  care  resources  developed.  Where  a  numicr 
or  brual  attack  has  taken  place,  then  the  police  are  appropriate;  where  uu 
overwhelmed  mother  has  a  child  who  continually  injures  himself  OIK-  to 
lack  of  supervision,  another  resouice  such  as  the  help  of  a  trained  home- 
maker  might  be  more  appropriate. 

Prevention  of  Child  Abuse 

To  save  a  child  from  the  serious  effects  and  irremedial  damage  caused 
by  abuse  and  neglect,  it  is  necessary  to  recognize  the  situation  when  il 
occurs.  Professionals  need  to  be  alert  when  they  notice  that  yomv! 
families  are  having  their  children  too  quickly,  with  no  relief  between 
pregnancies  The  danger  signs  of  marital  strain,  poverty,  isolation,  and 
overwhelmed  mothers  need  to  be  heeded.  Premature  births  with 
indicaoons  of  family  strain  should  be  of  great  concern;  all  possible 


XLn'V      suryeillfnce  should  be  g^en  these  parents.  Patents  with 
children  showing  developmental  difficulties  should  be  given  similar  holp. 

vJln  ™portant.  *at  education  be  restructured  so  that  every  you  up 
M?  «,£  ™hldf  >™  children  gr0w  and  devel°P-  Classes  should  I,S 
e  L^rii  ?*i        '  TWy  3t  the  elem«"*"y  and  junior  high   level 
nd    ?gnot  hi    hP,rOSPeCtlVe  m1°thers  Wil1  be  at  Icast  basically  informed 
S  6Xperience  with  child  c«rc-  U  cannot  bo 

^  t0°  many  women  thi»k  a  Xouna  baby  is 
i      CM  react  ^Wtectually  like  an  adult.  The 
pUnSlent  for  infants  «*^™  re-thinking  in 
development.  Again,  it  must   De 

Phenomenon,  ^^^  ,is  a/owor-cJ^ 

taught  the  importance  of  tJT      I  •?   "        classes'  Aud  males  nuist    >c 
and'the  ^^m^ntrAut^  to  *fi  stability  of  the  f  ami  ly 


^,made  -valbbte  if  we  are  ft, 

children.  Ap^pS^  ^  ^  "«"«*  thcir 
available.  Unwanted  children  ar^f  ?°ds  should  be  made  ensily 

long-term  residual  damages  ^  danger  of  ab«se  and  all   iu 


368 


Education  of  legal  personnel,  especially  that  of  judges,  is  necessary.  The 
investigators  note  that  even  in  cases  which  represented  blatant  abuse  as 
manifested  by  multiple  skeletal  trauma  with  central  nervous  system 
damage,  when  they  petitioned  for  removal  of  the  child  from  the  home  the 
authorities  were  more  concerned  about  the  rights  of  the  parents  than  the 
welfare  of  the  babies.  Some  children  were  returned  to  the  custody  of  their 
parents  after  their  fractures  had  healed  and  their  general  condition 
improved  without  any  assessment  of  the  family  at  all.  The  courts  do  not 
seem  to  be  truly  aware  of  the  risk  to  the  child  -  that  it  might  not  merely 
be  a  question  of  a  single  meaningless  act,  but  that  abuse  may  constitute  an 
active  expression  of  a  wish  to  be  rid  of  the  child.  The  overwhelming  odds 
against  complete  recovery  from  parental  abuse  seem  to  escape  appropriate 
attention.  The  horror  of  many  situations  and  the  intense  feelings  they 
arouse  may  cause  some  people  to  try  and  mend  the  parent-child 
relationship  in  order  to  wish  the  whole  situation  away.  It  is  not  always 
true  that  the  natural  parent  is  best  for  the  child,  nor  is  it  true  that  any 
parent  is  better  than  none.  Parents,  on  the  other  hand,  need  not  be  treated 
as  cuminals  because  they  have  abused  their  children.  A  total  assessment  of 
the  entire  situation  and  its  pressures  needs  to  be  made. 

One  issue,  that  of  community  support,  demands  special  attention.  As 
family  patterns  change  and  mobility  increases,  social  institutions  such  as 
the  church  lose  their  strong  hold  on  family  life.  People  who  tend  to  be 
isolated  become  even  more  so.  It  is  probably  true  that  many  parents  who 
do  not  beat  their  children  would  also  benefit  if  a  neighbor  could  help 
them  out  when  they  are  overwhelmed,  or  if  a  network  of  friendly  visitors 
would  somehow  fill  this  void.  Volunteers  could  be  used  to  extend  the 
work  of  the  public  health  nurses  and  the  hospital  clinics  to  ensure  that 
both  mild  supervision  and  help  would  be  available  to  young  mothers. 
Homemaker  services  can  be  extremely  important.  Community  programs 
and  neighborhood  associations  could  also  be  helpful  if  the  prohibition 
about  interfering  or  getting  involved  could  somehow  be  broken  down 
constructively.  It  is  possible  that  an  auxiliary  to  the  police  department 
could  be  useful. 

The  interrelationship  of  poverty,  isolation,  and  too  many 
Linplanned-for  small  children  is  important.  Too  often,  little  is  done  to 
reach  the  very  people  who  are  too  weak  to  ask  for  help.  Newspapers  and 
TV,  too,  often  carry  only  the  sensational  story  and  not  the  steady 
compilation  of  data  that  might  enable  us  to  make  reforms  in  our  welfare 
systems,  our  birth  control  clinics,  hospital  regulations,  courts,  and  foster 
home  programs  -  data  that  might  enable  us  to  prevent  such  tragedies  and 
save  these  children. 

Research  Grant:  MH  14739 
Date  of  Interview:  June  1969 

References: 

Caffey,  J.  Multiple  fractures  in  the  long  bones  of  infants  suffering  from  chronic 
subdural  hematoma.  American  Journal  of  Roentgenology,  August  1946. 

369 


uc  rrancis,  Vmcent  Child  Abuse  -  Preview  of  a  Nationwide  Survey.  The  Children's 

Division,  American  Humane  Association,  Denver,  Colorado,  1963. 
Elmer,  Elizabeth.  "Identification  of  Abused  Children."  Children.  U  S.  Department  of 

Health,  Education,  and  Welfare,  180-184,  September-October  1963. 
Elmer,  Elizabeth.  Hazards  in  determining  child  abuse  Child  Welfare,  January  1966. 
Elmer,  Elizabeth.  Child  abuse,  overview  of  the  problem  and  avenues  of  attack.  Paper 
presented  at  the  5th  Annual  Mental  Health  Institute,  St.  Louis,  Missouri,  July  1 966. 
Elmer,  Elizabeth.  Abused  children  and  community  resources.  JnternationalJournalof 

Offender  Therapy,  11,  1,  1967. 
Elmer,  Elizabeth  and  Gregg,  Grace,  M.D.  Developmental  characteristics  of  abused 

children.  Pediatrics,  40;4(Part  1):  596-602,  October  1967, 
Elmer,  Elizabeth  Children  in  Jeopardy  University  of  Pittsburgh  Press,  1967. 
Gregg,    Grace,    M.D.    Physician,    child-abuse    reporting    laws;    and    injured    child, 
psychosocial  anatomy   of  childhood   trauma,    Clinical  Pediatrics    Philadelphia, 
Pennsylvania;  J.  B.  Lippincott  Co.,  December  1968. 

Hill,  Reuben.  Social  stresses  on  the  family.  Social  Casework,  39:  139-150,  1958. 
Powell,  G.   F.,  M.D.,  Brasel,  J.  A.,  M.D.,  and   Blizzard,  R.  M.,  M.D.  Emotional 
deprivation  and  growth  retardation  simulating  idiopathic  hypopituitarisin:  I  - 
clinical  evaluation  of  the  syndrome.   The  New  England  Journal  of  Medicine, 
276,23:1271-1278,  June  8,  1967. 

Powell,  G.  F.,  M.D.,  Brasel,  J.  A.,  M.D  ,  Raiti,  S.,  M.D.,  and  Blizzard,  R.  M.,  M.D, 
Emotional  deprivation  and  growth  retardation  simulating  idiopathic 
hypopituitarism:  II  -  Endocrinologic  Evaluation  of  the  Syndrome!.  The  Journal  of 
the  American  Medical  Association,  88:358-362,  April  27,  1964. 
U  S.  Department  of  Health,  Education,  and  Welfare.  The  child  abuse  reporting  laws;  a 
tabular  view.  Washington,  D.C.,  1 966.  (Reprinted  with  revisions  in  1 968) 


370 


Investigator; 

John  M.  McKee,  Ph.  D. 

Draper  Correctional  Institute 

Elmore,  Ala. 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Introduction  and  Summary  of  Findings 

In  a  sparsely  settled  agricultural  area  25  miles  north  of  Montgomery, 
Ala.,  lies  a  unit  of  the  State's  prison  system  known  as  Draper  Correctional 
Center.  There,  some  800  men,  more  than  half  of  them  in  their  twenties  or 
late  teens,  are  serving  sentences  ranging  up  to  life  for  crimes  ranging  up  to 
murder. 

Under  the  Alabama  parole  system,  an  offender  is  usually  considered  for 
parole  when  he  has  completed  one-third  of  his  term.  (Inmates  joke  about 
Dne  of  their  fellows  who  is  serving  three  life  sentences  that  run 
consecutively.  "When  he  gets  through  the  first  one,"  the  joke  goes,  "they 
ire  going  to  have  a  -  of  a  time  getting  him  through  the  next  two." 
Actually  he  has  served  several  years  and  can  expect  a  chance  to  prove 
limself  in  the  outside  world  in  a  dozen  more.)  The  parole  system  helps  an 
Df fender  to  get  a  job;  it  requires,  in  fact,  that  he  have  a  job,  or  an  assured 
Mace  in  school,  before  he  is  released.  And  it  tries  to  keep  track  of  him  and 
:o  counsel  him.  Nevertheless,  it  is  estimated  that  from  65  to  70  percent  of 
lie  general  run  of  parolees  fall  into  criminal  ways  again  and  find 
Iiemselves  back  in  prison. 

Among  a  special  group  of  inmates,  however,  in  a  project  given  impetus 
iy  an  NIMH  grant,  a  preliminary  followup  indicates  that  only  about  30 
percent  of  those  released  have  had  to  conic  back. 

That  dramatic  cut  in  the  recidivism  rate  appears  to  have  been  achieved, 
n  great  part  at  least,  by  applying  principles  of  behavioral  science  to 
;pecific  educational  and  social  problems  of  inmates.  The  basic  tool  has 
icen  programed  instruction,  under  which  a  person  proceeds  at  his  own 
lace  through  a  given  subject,  whether  it  be  writing  grammatical  sentences 
)r  building  good  relations  on  the  job,  and  wins  rewards  as  he  goes  along. 
The  project  is  conducted  by  a  private  nonprofit  organization,  the 
Rehabilitation  Research  Foundation,  chartered  "by  the  State  to  develop 
ind  administer  programs  of  research  and  training  in  human  development. 
Offices  and  most  classrooms,  all  within  the  gates  at  Draper,  are  in  a 
varehouse-type  brick  building  that  once  served  the  prison  as  a  cotton  mill. 

Dr.  John  M.  McKee,  the  psychologist  who  conceived  the  program  and 
vho,  as  executive  director  of  the  Foundation,  administers  it,  points  out 

371 


that  the  participants  have  been  volunteers,  so  presumably  they  have  been 
more  highly  motivated  than  the  nonparticipants  to  improve  themselves 
and  to  make  good  in  the  outside  world.  Offsetting  this  at  least  to  sonw 
extent,  he  believes  that  many  and  perhaps  most  of  the  students  originally 
wanted  only  to  escape  work  on  the  prison  farm,  or  at  maintenance  jobs, 
or  in  the  laundry,  which  serves  the  entiie  State  prison  system. 

Besides  willingness  to  volunteer,  prospective  students  usually  have  liad 
to  demonstrate  a  sixth-grade  level  of  education,  which  was  considered 
essential  if  they  were  to  benefit  from  either  academic  or  vocational 
training.  Beginning  in  1968,  however,  applicants  below  this  level  have 
been  accepted  and  given  20  weeks  of  individual  and  group  instruction 
directed  toward  fitting  them  for  an  occupational  course.  Applicants' 
criminal  records  (except  in  the  case  of  rapists,  who  were  ruled  ineligible 
by  the  State  Board  of  Corrections  because  there  are  women  on  the 
Foundation's  staff)  and  their  records  of  behavior  in  the  institution  \\m 
not  in  themselves  affected  admissibility.  But  priority  has  gone  to  men 
having  a  reasonable  chance  of  being  paroled  soon  after  completion  of 
training. 

Doctor  McKee  traces  the  genesis  of  the  Draper  project  to  1959,  when 
he  was  one  of  a  group  of  psychologists  who  dreamed  of  establishing  a 
center  for  research  in  behavior,  with  emphasis  on  the  application  of 
learning  theory.  The  psychologists  were  staff  members  of  the  Division  of 
Mental   Hygiene  of  the  Alabama  State  Department  of  Health;  their 
behavior  center  would  be  part  of  the  University  of  Alabama,  and  one  of 
its  many  possible  research  studies  would  deal  with  inmates  of  Alabama 
prisons.  When  the  group  was  unable  to  raise  the  necessary  funds  for  the 
whole  program,  Doctor  McKee  decided  to  see  what  he  could  dp  with  (he 
correctional  project.  He  felt  certain,  after  several  years  of  directing  mentnl 
hygiene  work  in  the  State's  prison  system,  that  if  you  wanted  to  pry  a 
man  loose  from  antisocial  activities,  you  had  to  show  him  how  to  become 
and-more  important,  to  get  him  to  believe  that  he  could   becomc-a 
self-respecting  member  of  society.  Programed  instruction,  Doctor  McKcc 
believed,   offered   a  highly   promising  way   of  meeting  these   require- 
ments. 

Draper's  warden,  John  C.  Watkins,  who  had  majored  in  sociology  at  the 
University  of  Alabama  and  then  completed  course  work  for  a  masters 
degree  from  Auburn  University,  strongly  encouraged  Doctor  McKee  and 
offered  to  make  Draper  available  as  an  experimental  center  if  Alabama's 
Commissioner  of  Corrections  approved.  A.  Frank  Lee,  the  commissioner, 
not  only  approved  but  also  scraped  together  $3,000  to  help  finance  a  trial 
run  of  McKee's  idea.  ("We  sold  the  commissioner,"  McKee  recalls,  "by 
giving  him  a  little  programed  sequence  that  quickly  taught  him  how  to 
multiply  in  his  head  certain  2-digit  numbers  by  other  2-digit  numbers,") 
That  was  in  1961.  The  following  year,  NIMH  made  a  grant  to  expand 
the  work  and  analyze  the  results.  Institute  support,  which  continued  into 
1969,  went  mainly  to  develop,  test,  and  apply  self-instructional  programs 
in  the  academic  and  personal-relations  fields.  The  findings  were  so 
promising  that  in  1 964  the  Office  of  Education  and  the  Department  of 

372 


Labor  granted  funds,  under  the  Manpower  Development  and  Training  Act 
(MDTA),  to  enroll  more  inmates  and  to  add  vocational  subjects.  Work 
programs  under  the  NIMH  and  MDTA  grants  have  complemented  each 
other. 

For  the  most  part,  enrollees  now  spend  8  hours  a  day  in  the  program. 
There  is  considerable  variation  in  how  these  hours  are  apportioned.  The 
student  whose  aim  is  a  high  school  equivalency  certificate,  for  example, 
will  spend  most  of  his  time  in  academic  work.  Typically,  though,  two  of 
the  hours  are  given  to  basic  education  courses,  including  English,  remedial 
reading,  and  arithmetic,  and  the  rest  to  vocational  training— in  the 
classroom  for  theory  and  in  the  shop  for  practices.  Vocational  courses 
include  welding,  repair  of  electrical  appliances  (including  air  conditioning 
and  refrigeration  systems),  barbering,  sign  writing,  and  work  in  an 
automobile  service  station. 

Among  the  regular  staff  membeis,  in  addition  to  Doctor  McKee,  are 
Donna  M.  Seay,  a  vocational  educator  who  serves  as  the  Foundation's 
assistant  director;  four  learning  managers  or  supervisors,  who  in  a 
conventional  educational  system  would  be  called  teachers;  five  vocational 
instructors;  and  three  counselors,  for  guidance  to  men  in  the  program,  for 
job  placement,  and  for  followup. 

The  staff  is  augmented  by  the  so-called  college  corps-junior  and  senior 
college  students,  majoring  in  sociology,  psychology,  or  education,  who 
join  the  regular  staff  during  summer  vacations  or  for  part  of  a  semester 
during  the  school  year.  Some  50  corpsmen  from  a  dozen  colleges  and 
universities  in  Alabamp  and  neighboring  States  have  worked  at  Draper, 
three  or  four  at  any  given  time.  They  have  served  as  counselors,  as  aides  to 
the  regular  instructors,  and,  perhaps  most  important,  as  "peer 
models"— examples  of  the  fact  that  a  young  man  can  do  something 
worthwhile  with  his  life.  "With  encouragement  from  the  corpsmen," 
Doctor  McKee  reports,  "many  inmates  came  to  believe  for  the  first  time 
that  their  lives  counted  and  that  they,  too,  could  succeed." 

About  five  "service"  corpsmen  also  are  on  duty,  mainly  as  assistants  to 
the  instructors.  They  are  relatively  well-educated  inmates  who  have  shown 
special  interest  in  the  project  and  the  desire  and  ability  to  work  with 
members  of  the  regular  staff  and  with  fellow  inmates. 

Augmenting  the  academic  and  vocational  work,  the  project  has  set  up: 

1.  A  reading  laboratory,  which  evaluates  and  provides  help  for  each 
student  who  seems  deficient  in  reading  skills.  Some  of  the  work— notably, 
rate-comprehension    exercises     presented     by    a    variable-speed     film 
projector— is  clone  in  groups,  but  most  of  it  is  individual.  Each  student 
receives  a  kit  that  contains  stones  and  self-graded  tests  on  the  content  of 
the    stories.    Vocabularies    are    developed    through    other    self-graded 
exercises.  Students  are  encouraged  to  spend  at  least  an  hour  a  day  outside 
the  laboratory  reading  any  library  book  of  their  choice,  provided  it  is  not 
far  below  their  known  level  of  ability. 

2.  A   seminar  program,    which  provides   the  group  interaction  not 
present   in    the  programed   instruction   courses.   Group  discussion,   or 
seminars,    have    dealt    so    far   with    English   composition,    poetry 

373 


appieciation,  human  behavioi,  great  books,  creative  writing,  and 
events.  Most  of  the  leaders  have  been  volunteers  fiom  Montgomery 
recruited  by  Doctor  McKee  because  of  their  interest  in  the  subject  matter, 
The  program  has  been  so  successful  in  awakening  and  expanding  the 
prisoner's  interests  that  it  will  be  continued  and  broadened. 

About  100  inmates  were  enrolled  in  the  Draper  project  as  of  mid-1969* 
bringing  the  grand  total  of  participants-including  a  number  in  the  early 
years  who  were  involved  for  only  short  periods-to  almost  2,000.  Better 
than  40  percent  of  the  current  enrollees  are  Negroes,  a  proportion  that  has 
more  than  doubled  since  the  start  of  the  program.  Two  thirds  of  the 
institution's  population  is  white. 

Doctor  McKee  reports  the  following  major  findings,  most  of  them 
based  on  a  followup  of  228  graduates  of  the  vocational  training  program 
(out  of  290  who  had  been  released)  from  1  to  3  years  after  they  hud  left 
Draper. 

•  Sixty-eight  percent  of  those  released-most  of  them  by  parole,  the 
others  by  completion  of  their  terms-were  still  free.  Of  the  other  32 
percent,  almost  half  had  been  returned  to  Draper  for  technical  violations, 
such  as  failure  to  keep  in  touch  with  parole  authorities,  or  consorting  with 
known  criminals,  while  the  rest  had  been  picked  up  for  new  felonies  or 
misdemeanors.  This  contrasts  with  the  pretraining  record  of  these  men: 
fully  70  percent  had  been  recidivists  and  only  30  percent  had  been  first 
offendeis.  For  the  State  as  a  whole  the  recidivism  rate  was  estimated  at 
between  50  and  70  percent. 

•  Of  the  288  men  in  the  followup,  about  80  percent  had  initially  gone 
into  jobs  directly  related  to  their  training  and  another  10  percent  had 
gone  into  such  jobs  later  on.  The  number  of  employers  who  hired 
graduates  reached  361,  since  some  graduates  changed  jobs  several  times. 
Two-thirds  of  those  released  got  jobs  through  the  efforts  of  the  program's 
job  development  and  placement  officers.  Only  a  few  graduates-eight  out 
of    the    187    for   whom   this   information    was   available— performed 
unsatisfactorily  in  their  first  jobs, 

•  The  average  income  of  the  trainees  who  have  been  released— based  on 
follow-up  data  from  150-is  $3,640  a  year.  Each  man  pays  an  estimated 
average  of  $546  a  year  in  taxes.  Moreover,  the  public  has  been  relieved  of 
the  burden  of  maintaining  the  man  in  prison,  estimated  at  $1 ,200  per  man 
per  year. 

•  After  200  hours  of  programed  instruction,  students  have  shown  an 
average  gain  in  educational  level  of  1.4  grades.  When  this  instruction  was 
coupled  with  a  reading  improvement  course,  the  gain  was  2.5  grades, 

•  In  the  hope  of  getting  a  certificate  of  high  school  equivalency,  178 
students  have  taken  the  State's  General  Education  Development  tests,  and 
all  except  nine  have  passed  the  tests  and  received  the  certificate.  These 
students  typically  had  been  at  the  seventh  grade  level  or  even  below  in  at 
least  some  of  their  subjects  when  they  enrolled.  (Incidentally,  evidence  of 
the  cash  value  of  a  high  school  education  was  turned  up  by  a  1 967  study 
of  one  group  of  former  inmates.  Men  who  had  earned  a  certificate  of  high 
school  equivalency  during  their  imprisonment  were  making  an  average  of 

374 


$349,30  a  month;  the  others  were  making  £279.  As  compared  with 
pre-irnprisonment  earnings,  the  high  school  equivalency  group  had  added 
more  than  $140  a  month;  the  others,  less  than  $40.) 

•  Thirteen  students  were  accepted  by  colleges  while  they  were  still 
inmates,  and  entered  the  colleges  upon  parole.  Three  of  these  have 
graduated;  the  others  are  still  in  college.  Most  of  the  students  have 
received  help  from  a  scholarship  loan  fund  called  PACE  (Program  for 
Achievement  of  a  College  Education),  which  the  Foundation  administers. 
The  fund  was  started  in  1962  when  the  inmates  who  had  taken  part  in  one 
of  the  early  progiamed  instruction  experiments,  for  which  they  were 
paid,  donated  their  earnings  to  establish  it.  Since  then  it  has  been 
suppoited  by  gifts  from  outside.  Loans  to  college  students,  graduates  of 
the  Diaper  program,  have  ranged  from  $40  to  over  $2,000.  The  fund  has 
had  to  turn  down  some  requests  and  meet  others  only  in  small  part. 

To  disseminate  information  about  its  accomplishments  and  methods, 
the  Foundation  has  held  four  conferences—in  Montgomery,  Houston 
(with  the  University  of  Houston  as  cosponsor),  New  York  City  (with  the 
Staten  Island  Mental  Health  Center  as  cosponsor),  and  Berkeley,  Calif, 
(with  the  University  of  California  as  cosponsor).  Each  was  attended  by 
more  than  100  persons  from  correctional  institutions,  pardon  and  parole 
boards,  State  employment  services,  State  manpower  development  and 
training  programs,  and  vocational  rehabilitation  agencies.  More  than  2,000 
persons  have  visited  the  project  for  first-hand  information.  A  manual  on 
Draper's  training  methods  has  been  prepared. 

Correctional  institutions  in  a  number  of  States— including  California, 
Florida,  Georgia,  Illinois,  Indiana,  Nevada,  Oklahoma,  South  Carolina,  and 
Tennessee-have  adopted  some  part  of  the  Draper  program,  and  others 
have  expressed  interest  in  doing  so.  For  lack  of  funds,  Doctor  McKee 
reports,  Alabama  has  not  yet  been  able  to  extend  the  program  beyond 
Draper. 


Programed  Instruction 

At  the  basis  of  the  program  is  learning  theory,  or  reinforcement  theory, 
which  says  that  if  desirable  behavior  is  rewarded  and  undesirable  behavior 
ignored  or  punished,  a  person's  behavior  will  eventually  be  modified  in  the 
desired  direction. 

"There  is  no  substitute  in  this  world  for  competence,"  says  Doctor 
McKee.  "And  that  is  what  these  trainees  have  lacked  all  their  lives— a 
feeling  of  competence.  They  have  failed  in  every  major  task  they  have 
undertaken,  perhaps  with  good  reason:  the  family  may  have  failed  them, 
or  the  school.  As  the  warden  says,  they  have  even  failed  in  crime,  or  they 
wouldn't  be  here." 

Programed  instruction,  or  P.I.,  is  intended  to  substitute  success  for 
failure.  As  defined  by  Doctor  McKee,  it  is  "an  attempt  to  systematically 

375 


01    learning   theory    to   an   individual   learning 
situation."  Subject  matter  is  said  to  be  programed  when: 

1.  Its  training  objectives  are  behavioral:  that  is,  the  learne-r  will  be  able 
to  do  something  specific. 

2.  Material  is  presented  in  a  logical  sequence  of  steps-called  frames  or 
exercises-each  small  enough  to  be  easily  learned. 

3.  The  student  is  requited  to  respond  actively  to  each  step;  that  is,  he  is 
asked  to  apply  information  or  exercise  skills  immediately  after  they  are 
introduced. 

4.  The  learner  receives  immediate  feedback;  that  is,  he  is  told  at  once 
that  he  is  right  or  wiong.  In  good  P.I.  he  is  almost  always  right.  TJife 
explains  the  behavior-shaping  power  of  P.I.,  says  Doctor  McKee,  because 
the  knowledge  that  a  response  is  right  is  reinforcing  to  the  student:  he  is 
encouraged  to  go  ahead.  If  he  is  wrong,  he  is  told  why  and  may  be 
referred  to  a  section  that  helps  him  over  the  difficult  part.  He  is  asked  to 
conect  his  answer;  then  he  is  moved  forward  in  the  program, 

A  course  on  sentence  structure,  for  instance,  begins  by  defining  subject 
and  predicate  and  giving  examples.  Then  it  offers  a  few  simple  sentences 
and  asks  the  student  to  pick  out  the  subject  and  predicates.  It  tells  him 
the  answers.  If  he  has  been  right,  he  goes  on  to  more  complex  sentences;  if 
wrong,  he  reviews  the  definitions  and  corrects  his  mistakes  before  going 
ahead. 

One  series  of  lessons  teaches  personal  hygiene  as  an  important  factor  in 
getting  and  holding  a  job.  Other  lessons  try  to  teach  desirable  work  habits 
and  customer-relations  attitudes.  Here  is  one  unit,  or  frame,  from  the 
customer-relations  course  for  automobile  station  workers; 

Make  the  first  move  count.-  J.  Q.  Customer  drives  into  your  shop, 
This  is  his  first  visit,  and  he  feels  a  bit  wary.  The  longer  he  is  left 
waiting,  the  more  doubtful  he  becomes.  He  may  even  decide  that 
you're  not  interested  in  his  business  and  leave. 

*  *  *  But  no!  You're  on  your  toes!  You  see  that  no  one  has  made 
a  move  to  help  him.  At  that  particular  moment  you  cannot  leave 
your  work,  so  you  call  out,  "Good  morning,  sir!  Someone  will  be 
with  you  in  a  moment."  He  at  least  knows  now  that  someone  is 
aware  of  his  presence  and  is  hurrying  to  assist  him.  The  sooner  you 
can  approach  him,  the  better. 

Put  a  check  mark  by  the  following  statements  that  are  true; 

\ ,  Most  customers  don't  mind  waiting,  • — 

2.  If  the  customer  is  kept  waiting  too  long,  he  may  leave.       — — • 

3.  You  should  approach  the  customer  as  soon  as  possible,       , . 

4.  Keep  the  customer  waiting  for  a  while  to  show  him  how  hard 

everyone  is  working. 

5.  If  it  is  not  possible  to  approach  the  customer  immediately,  tell 

him  that  someone  will  be  with  him  as  soon  as  possible. 

You  scored  100  percent  if  you  checked  numbers  2,3,  and  5,  You 
realized  that  most  people  do  not  appreciate  being  kept  waiting.  Time 
is  important  to  all  of  us,  and  no  one  likes  his  time  to  be  wasted.  Too, 

376 


a  customer  will  be  more  impressed  with  your  attention  to  his  needs 
than  he  will  be  with  your  efforts  to  fix  someone  else's  car! 

If  you  checked  a  wrong  answer  or  failed  to  check  the  correct 
answer,  go  back  and  correct  your  answer  before  continuing. 

In  the  Draper  project,  a  learning  area  replaces  the  usual  classroom.  It  is 
a  room  with  rows  of  stalls  or  cubicles,  like  those  found  in  college  library 
stacks,  so  that  each  student  has  his  own,  compartmentalized,  study  space. 
A  dozen  or  15  students  work  in  the  same  area  under  the  eye  of  a  learning 
manager  or  supervisor,  but  no  two  students  are  likely  to  be  engaged  on  the 
same  subject.  The  supervisor  answers  questions,  offers  encouragement, 
and  keeps  records. 

Programed  instruction  makes  it  easy  to  woik  with  a  person  at  several 
levels,  depending  on  his  need.  One  student,  for  example,  is  relatively  far 
advanced  in  English,  so  he  is  studying  it  at  the  eighth  grade  level.  Since  he 
is  extremely  weak  in  arithmetic,  howevei,  he  studies  that  subject  at  the 
fifth  grade  level. 

P.I.  materials  come  either  in  text  form  or  for  use  in  teaching  machines. 
The  machines  have  advantages.  They  can  automatically  skip  ovei  material 
that  the  student's  responses  show  he  already  knows,  and  they  can  present 
extra  practice  sequences  when  the  response  indicates  these  are  needed. 
Further,  they  can  control  cheating  and  can  tabulate  the  number  of  correct 
and  incorrect  responses,  thus  easing  the  supervisor's  job.  The  Draper 
project  uses  them  to  some  extent  but,  largely  in  the  interest  of  economy, 
has  relied  mainly  on  texts,  or  "software,"  and  feels  the  results  have  been 
good. 

In  the  beginning,  the  available  P.I.  courses  covered  only  a  few  subjects 
and  often  lacked  what  Doctor  McICee  considered  a  necessity; 
self-administered  tests  that  evaluated  the  subject's  knowledge  at  the  start, 
at  various  points  along  the  way,  and  at  the  end.  So  the  project  set  to  work 
to  develop  materials  of  its  own.  Some  30  Draper-tested  courses  covering 
aspects  of  such  basic  subjects  as  grammar  and  mathematics,  such 
vocational  subjects  as  electronics,  welding,  auto  mechanics,  and 
bricklaying,  and  such  social  relations  areas  as  applying  for  a  job,  good  job 
habits,  and  table  manners  have  resulted;  they  are  on  sale  through  the 
University  of  Alabama. 

The  development  of  its  own  courses  went  hand  in  hand  with  the  use  of 
existing  ones.  One  of  these,  used  purely  because  it  did  exist,  was  Russian 
language  for  beginners.  But  could  this  possibly  have  any  value  to  prison 
inmates?  "Yes  indeed,  it  could  and  did."  answers  McKee.  "The  fellows 
who  took  it  went  back  and  spouted  off  a  few  words  in  Russian  to  one 
another.  And  when  the  others  asked  'What's  that?1,  our  boys  said,  'If  you 
went  to  The  School,  you'd  find  out.'  Their  new  knowledge  gave  them 
prestige,  and  that's  important." 

Some  of  the  students  began  programing  on  their  own.  One  day,  a  few 
months  after  the  project  began,  two  inmates— call  them  Ted  Jones  and  Bill 
Smith— came  to  McKec  and  said,  "Hello,  Doc,  take  a  look  at  this." 
McKee  looked  and  saw  a  sheaf  of  handwritten  instructions  and  diagrams 
entitled  "A  Programed  Course  in  Fingering  Movements  on  the  Trumpet. 

377 


By  Ted  Jones  and  Bill  Smith."  It  had  1,164  frames.  Leafing  through  if, 
McKee  remarked,  "Gee,  I  believe  I  could  take  this  course  and  learn  to  play 
the  trumpet.  It  even  teaches  you  how  to  blow  through  the  trumpet.  You 
blow  as  if  you  were  going  to  spit  a  piece  of  tobacco  off  your  tongue/' 

"Well,"  Ted  Jones  answered,  "if  you  are  as  smart  as  the  four  men  we've 
already  put  through  the  program  to  test  it,  and  at  the  end  could  play 
those  three  tunes-yeah,  if  you  are  as  smart  as  they  are,  you  could  Ictnn  to 
play  those  tunes,  too." 

"Another  year  and  a  day  for  you,"  McKee  joked  back,  and  went  on  to 
encourage  Ted  and  Bill  to  continue  their  work,  which  they  did,  writing 
lessons  for  the  clarinet  and  saxophone  and  a  basic  course  in  music 
notation  (One  of  the  musicians  is  now  in  college;  the  other  has  been 
returned  to  Draper  for  a  parole  violation.) 

Over  the  years  other  inmates  have  developed  courses  in  personal 
grooming,  etiquette,  parts  of  speech,  improving  the  memory,  orientation 
for  new  prisoners,  and  other  subjects.  One  man  who  was  studying 
technical  writing  signed  up  for  a  half-year  course  in  welding  just  so  lie 
could  program  lessons  in  the  theory  of  it.  The  course  in  technical  writing, 
incidentally,  was  diopped  after  a  few  years  because  employers  shied  away 
from  hiring  its  graduates.  But  the  welding  lessons,  like  most  of  the  other 
courses  developed  by  inmates,  have  become  part  of  the  Draper 
curriculum. 

Meanwhile  the  commercial  production  of  P.T.  materials  has  boomed  to 
such  an  extent  that  hundreds  of  courses  are  available.  The  problem  fit 
Draper  is  no  longer  to  develop  new  courses  but  to  select  from  the  most 
promising  ones  at  hand,  One  new  program,  though,  is  definitely  planned. 
It  will  be  written  with  the  aid  of  offenders-in  particular,  successful  iiml 
unsuccessful  parolees-and  will  teach  how  to  succeed  on  parole. 


Reinforcement 

Although  programed  instruction  has  its  own  built-in  reinforce ment-Ehe 
lift  one  gets  from  finding  that  an  answer  is  correct  (or,  if  it  isn't  correct, 
from  learning  what  was  wrong  and  from  being  encouraged  to  go 
ahead)-the  staff  at  Draper  found  that  the  typical  inmate  got  bored  after  a 
few  weeks.  So  it  has  employed  a  number  of  tangible  rewards  to  create  a 
motivating  environment.  For  instance,  students  who  complete  a  course  arc 
given  points,  and  enough  points  win  them  a  Certificate  of  Achievement, 
Along  with  written  recommendations,  these  certificates  provide  a  stiong 
basis  for  favorable  parole  consideration.  Many  former  inmates  have 
reported  that  the  certificates  also  helped  them  to  get  jobs. 

Now  undergoing  trial  is  a  "contingency  management"  plan  under  which 
a  student  contracts  at  the  end  of  each  day's  classroom  work  to  complete  ii 
certain  number  of  frames  the  next  day.  A  frame  comprises  one  question 
and  one  answer  in  a  programed  instruction  course,  A  contract  is  an 
agreement,  recorded  on  a  form,  between  the  student  and  the  supervisor. 
The  student  is  free  to  specify  the  number  of  frames  he  will  do  provided 

378 


this  is  at  least  equal  to  his  accomplishment  during  the  baseline  period  at  the 
beginning  of  the  course.  The  student  also  agrees  to  take  the  tests  assigned 
by  the  supervisor  at  the  appropriate  points  in  the  course.  And  student  and 
supervisor  agree  that  upon  completion  of  a  certain  number  of  frames  or  of 
a  test  the  student  will  be  free  to  take  a  15-minute  break.  {The  amount  of 
work  to  be  completed  before  a  break  depends  upon  the  total  amount  for 
the  day  as  specified  in  the  contract.)  The  agreement  makes  it  possible  for 
each  student  to  average  one  break  an  hour. 

The  breaks  are  taken  in  a  room- the  "reinfoicing  event"  area-fitted 
out  with  a  coffeemaker  and  supplies,  newspapers,  books,  magazines, 
letter-writing  materials,  shoe-shining  equipment,  crossword  puzzles, 
playing  cards,  checker  and  chess  sets,  and  a  radio. 

An  experiment  that  preceded  the  adoption  of  this  plan  involved  16 
students,  First  they  worked  through  a  3-week  baseline  period  having  two 
scheduled  breaks  per  day.  Then  came  4  weeks  managed  by  the 
experimenter.  During  the  first  two  of  these  weeks  he  set  the  performance 
level  at  20  percent  higher  than  the  baseline  average;  during  the  last  2 
weeks,  he  raised  it  another  20  percent.  Most  students  achieved  these 
increases. 

Then  came  2  weeks  of  self-management,  with  the  students  themselves 
at  the  end  of  each  day  setting  the  amount  of  work  they  would  do  the 
following  day.  In  almost  every  case  they  agreed  to  work  and  did  work 
faster  than  during  the  second  phase,  when  the  experimenter  was  calling 
the  pace.  Frames  completed  per  hour  averaged  61  during  the  baseline 
period,  101  during  the  experimenter-managed  period,  and  125  during  the 
self-management  period. 

These  increases  in  work  per  hour  were  accompanied  by  a  decrease  in 
the  number  of  hours  worked.  During  the  second  and  third  periods,  the 
students  were  permitted  to  leave  the  study  area  when  they  had  completed 
the  stipulated  or  agreed-upon  number  of  frames  and  the  appropriate  tests. 
They  could  go  back  to  the  dormitory  and  rest,  work  out  in  the  gyrn,  talk 
to  friends,  or  engage  in  other  free-time  activities.  Consequently,  the 
number  of  hours  worked  per  day  dropped  from  slightly  more  than  5  in 
the  baseline  period,  to  4.35  in  the  experimenter-managed  period,  to  3.4  in 
the  self -management  phase.  But  the  total  work  completed  rose  from  320 
frames  in  the  baseline  period,  to  405,  to  435.  The  increase  in  frames 
completed  did  not  adversely  affect  test  performance. 

In  sum,  the  experiment  indicates: 

1 .  The  knowledge  that  a  reward  is  waiting  spurs  students  on.  This  is  so 
whether  the  work  to  be  done  is  set  by  the  experimenter  or  by  the  student. 
But  when  it  is  set  by  the  student,  productivity  seems  to  increase  still 
further. 

2.  In  many  cases,  permission  to  leave  the  experimental  area  at  the 
completion  of  the  performance  contract  for  the  day  acted  as  a  more 
powerful  reinforcer  than  the   immediate  reward-one  or  more  of  the 
activities   available  during  the   15 -minute-break  periods.  After  students 
were  told,  part  way  through  the  experiment,  that  they  might  continue  to 

379 


study  rather  than  take  an  eained  break,  the  gioup  as  a  whole  chose 
continued  work  as  often  as  it  chose  immediate  relaxation. 

In  addition  to  the  built-in  leinforcement  of  programed  instruction  ami 
the  extrinsic,  tangible  rewards  offered  at  Draper,  something  else  is  al 
work.  In  the  process  of  learning  to  be  a  student,  Doctor  McKee  obseives,  a 
man  learns  to  enjoy  certain  intellectual  pursuits.  He  reads  the  newspapers, 
he  picks  up  a  book,  he  listens  to  the  news  on  TV,  he  carries  on  iin 
intellectual  conversation— his  whole  world  is  changing  and  opening  up,  lie 
is  going  from  such  reinforcers  as  a  party,  free  time,  and  points  towaid  a 
certificate  to  find  intrinsic  rewards  in  the  subject  matter  itself  and  in  tliu 
process  of  his  development.  Tlu's  is  theory,  but  you  see  it  working  in  the 
lives  of  all  of  us. 

The  attitude  adopted  by  the  staff  is  also  a  motivating  factor,  Doctor 
McKee  believes.  "We  attempt  to  be  firm  but  not  punitive,"  he  says, 
"helping  but  not  totally  permissive,  and  flexible  but  not  vacillating.  Jty 
employing  positive  rather  than  negative  reinforcement  whenever  possible, 
an  atmosphere  had  been  created  that  is  much  different  from  the  hostile, 
punitive  one  expected  by  most  inmates." 

As  an  example,  he  cites  his  own  behavior  when  he  helps  administei  a 
test  for  a  high  school  equivalency  certificate.  It's  a  long  test,  dragging  on 
for  several  hours.  Since  the  typical  inmate's  span  of  attention  is  short,  and 
since  McKee  knows  from  experience  that  the  inmate  may  say  "The  -with 
it"  and  walk  off,  the  director  keeps  coming  back  and  showing  interest. 
"How  are  you  domg-any  pioblems?"  he'll  ask.  "I  don't  tell  him  any 
answers,"  McKee  says,  "but  I  get  him  to  say,  'Well,  yeah,  this  part  I  just 
took,'  or  something  like  that.  And  I  say,  'Well,  let  me  see,'  and  look  at  the 
part  and  say,  'Yes,  you've  got  some  tough  questions  there.  Better  go  hack 
over  what  you've  been  reading  and  writing  to  make  sure  you've  got  the 
light  answers.  1*11  be  back.'  "  What  I'm  trying  to  do  is  to  maintain  alert 
behavior-keep  them  sticking  with  the  job  and  not  bugging  out. 

"That's  what  they've  been  doing  all  their  Jives-bugging  out,  escaping. 
And  strangely  enough,  this  escape  behavior  has  been  reinforced.  Rob  a 
house,  forge  a  check,  steal  a  car-and  run  away.  Girl  friend  squeezing  you 
in?  The  job  getting  you  down?  Run  away. 

"Even  people  in  correctional  work  have  been  reinforcing  such  behavior. 
An  inmate  will  get  tired  of  the  work  he's  been  assigned  to.  Or  he'll  get 
tired  of  the  person  he's  working  with.  So  he  starts  manipulating  a  change. 
He'll  tell  the  classification  officer:  'I'm  bugging  out  of  this.  I  need  a 
different  job.  My  supervisor's  down  on  me-I  don't  know  why.'  And  hell 
keep  trying  and  after  a  while  he'll  get  his  change." 

From  staff  interviews  with  prospective  students,  McKee  suspects  that  a 
large  proportion  of  the  inmates  who  volunteered  for  school  were  simply 
running  away  once  again.  But  he  accepted  them  because  in  his  project 
there  is  nowhere  to  run  to,  except  back  to  the  job  they  escaped  from. 

380 


A  "Second  Language" 

Doctor  McKee  emphasizes  that  with  prisoneis-and  probably  any  other 
population,  for  that  matter-programed  instruction  has  to  be 
supplemented  by  discussion  groups  and  sometimes  by  individual 
counseling  or  teacher-student  conferences.  The  inescapable  inadequacy  of 
many  P.I.  courses,  standing  by  themselves,  was  brought  home  to  him  one 
day  a  few  years  ago  when  he  congratulated  a  trainee  on  having  made  the 
extraordinarily  high  grade  of  96  in  a  high-school-level  grammar  course. 
The  trainee  gave  a  deprecatory  smile  in  response  and  said:  'This  here 
English,  Doc,  don't  give  me  no  trouble  nohow."  Like  many  other 
students,  he  had  learned  the  rules  but  wasn't  applying  them. 

The  project  then  tried  the  seminar  approach-trainees  meeting  in  groups 
with  a  teacher  and  working  together  at  speech  modification.  This  was 
much  more  successful.  As  the  trainees  listened  to  one  another,  they 
learned  to  detect  errors,  which  they  called  to  the  group's  attention. 

Now  the  staff  is  experimenting  with  what  it  calls  the  "second  language" 
approach;  that  is,  teaching  standard  English  by  the  oral  methods  used  in 
teaching  a  foreign  language. 

In  this  approach  a  man's  present  speech,  no  matter  how  poor  by 
conventional  standards,  is  never  criticized.  It  is  the  one  he  has  picked  up 
as  a  child,  and  it  is  usually  quite  effective  in  meeting  his  needs  to 
communicate  with  his  usual  associates.  The  trainees  are  simply  told  that 
the  school  wants  to  give  them  the  ability  to  use  another  kind  of  speech  on 
appropriate  occasions,  such  as  when  applying  for  a  job  or  meeting  a  girl 
friend's  parents.  "We  all  have  different  levels  of  speech,"  points  out  Sally 
Roy,  a  former  public  school  teacher  who  is  now  a  research  associate  with 
the  Diaper  project.  "A  child  of  five  will  speak  to  his  mother  one  way,  to 
his  friends  another,  and  to  his  teacher  a  third.  We  speak  one  way  to  the 
dog  and  another  to  the  Mayor.  The  hope  is  that  the  trainees  will  use  this 
different  level  we  are  giving  them  more  and  more  often  and  that 
eventually  it  will  become  their  usual  level." 

The  project  got  the  idea  from  St.  Mary's  Dominican  College  Business 
School  in  New  Orleans,  a  basic  education  center  for  the  Job  Corps.  In 
training  Negro,  Cajun,  and  other  disadvantaged  girls  to  be  secretaries,  staff 
members  from  Draper  found,  Dominican  College  was  trying  not  to  correct 
their  language  but  to  give  them  a  new  one  for  use  on  the  job.  It  called  the 
new  language  "business  speech." 

In  its  second-language  experiment,  the  staff  at  Draper  gives  extensive 
drill  work  on  the  most  common  errors  noticed  during  talks  with  the 
trainees— errors  exemplified  by  such  sentences  as  "John  and  me  want  to 
go  to  town,"  "He  don't  know  no  better,"  and  "They  is  ready  to  fight," 
The  trainees  hear  over  and  over  again,  on  tape,  the  correctly  spoken 
version  of  these  and  hundreds  of  other  examples.  They  also  listen  to 
themselves  at  frequent  intervals.  And  there  is  group  work,  including  mock 
job  interviews,  some  of  it  recorded  by  a  videotape  machine. 

Has  any  employer  said,  "I  can't  take  this  man  because  he  doesn't  talk 
well  enough?" 

381 


, .  -_  ~.  itvw  uiieans,"  answers  Doctor  McKee.  "People  (lie« 
said,  l\Ve  can't  take  tliis  girl;  she  doesn't  speak  right;  she'd  give  us  ab:l 
image/  We  don't  know  whether  or  not  it  has  happened  here.  But  i!  SLYJI> 
reasonable  to  suppose  that  an  employer  hires  a  man  on  the  basis  of  tto 
total  Image  he  casts,  including  his  ability  and  his  self-confidence,  and  ilui 
his  speech  is  part  of  this  image.  Even  an  employer  who  himself  snys  Ik 
don't'  cannot  help  being  impressed  by  a  man  who  speaks  wcff 
Rehabilitation  is  a  cluster  of  things,  including  attitude  changes.  We  tliinl 
that  proper  speech  is  part  of  the  clustei.  It  is  not  merely  that  the  man  wlio 
learns  what  we  are  calling  a  second  language  has  acquired  a  new  skill  but 
that  he  is  enhanced  by  having  done  so.  People  look  twice  at  him  urni  think 
that  he  is  worth  more.  He  himself  has  a  greater  feeling  of  worth  ami  a 
sense  of  belonging." 


For  the  Future 

Aided  by  a  new  grant  from  the  Department  of  Labor,  Doctor  McKw 
expects  to  continue  the  program  of  education  and  vocational  training  urnl 
to  make  more  extensive  and  rigorous  followups.  He  hopes  to  answer  thesu 
questions,  among  others: 

»  What  is  the  most  effective  type  of  training  program,  as  measured  by 
work  adjustment  and  recidivism?  Can  P.I,  simulate  real  work  situations, 
beyond  that  of  a  job  interview?  Can  realistic  work-stress  conditions  be 
simulated  and  the  means  of  handling  them  be  transferred  to  nclunt  job 
conditions  following  release? 

•  What    specific   barriers,    particularly    those    related    to    employe* 
attitudes,  make  it  more  difficult  for  the  offender  to  find  and  hold  it  job? 

*  Do  released  convicts  who  receive  "labor  mobility"  funds  (siiwH 
grants  of  Federal  money  to  tide  them  over  until  the  first  paycheck  conies 
in)   or  Federal  bonding  assistance,  or  both,  have  a  better  post-release 
record  than  those  who  do  not? 

»  How  can  an  inmate's  behavior,  not  merely  during  the  hours  given  to 
the  training  program  but  throughout  the  day,  be  shaped  to  conform  to 
desired  standards?  Involved  are  (a)  deciding  which  kinds  of  behavior  arc 
desirable,  because  they  make  for  a  successful  transition  from  the 
institution  to  a  job  in  the  community,  and  which  kinds  of  behavior  are 
undesirable,  (bj  determining  the  frequency  with  which  each  kind  of 
behavior  occurs  m  the  sample  of  men  to  be  studied,  and  (c)  selecting, 
applying,  and  measuring  the  effectiveness  of  measures-am ong  them, 
probably,  certain  changes  in  the  attitudes  of  the  custodial  staff-to 
encourage  one  kind  of  behavior  and  discourage  another.  This,  then,  is  n 
major,  long-term  experiment  in  behavior  modification  and  one  that  is  dear 
to  Doctor  McKee's  heart. 


382 


Research  Grant.    MH  14990 

Date  of  Interview.    December  1968 

References 

Clements,    C.   B.,   and    McKee,  J.   M.  Programed   instruction   for   institutionalized 

offenders'    contingency  management  and  performance   contracts.  Psychological 

Reports,  22:957-964,  1968. 
McKee,  J.  M,  Adult  basic  education  for  the  disadvantaged:  procedures  used  to  raise  the 

basic  educational  level.  Presented  to  the  American  Vocational  Association,  1966. 
McKee,   J .   M.   Programed   instruction   in    the   correctional   process.    Presented  at 

Conference  in  Manpower  Training  for  Offenders  in  the  Correctional  Process,  1968. 


383 


Investigator. 

James  F.  Short,  Jr.,  Ph.D.* 
University  of  Chicago 
Chicago,  III. 


Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Members  of  juvenile  delinquent  gangs,  according  to  a  theory  atlviinc 
m  an  important  new  study,  suffer  from  a  condition  described  us  ,mo 
disability.  Much  more  so  than  other  boys,  they  grow  to  school  ii 
without  the  skills  needed  to  meet  new  situations.  Thus  Uiey  ;i 
handicapped  both  in  getting  along  with  teachers  and  classmates  and 
earning,  and  their  school  experiences  in  turn  make  them  additional 
Handicapped  for  conventional  activities  later  on,  including  work.  In  t 
effort  to  meet  the  universal  need  for  relationships  with  other  peopif,  tin 
drift  into  gang  membership.  it* 

t0  3  co?Pleme"tary  theory  advanced  in  the  same  slmh 
/    3  gf"gs  delmcluent  ac«vities  may  be  likened  to  playing 
*  Siight  reward  bllt  j»  which  also  a  MOM, 
may  be  exacted«  If  »c«<>n  is  coming  u 


is  termed  the  ,/wory  ,, 

presses  d  gang  memb°'-s' 


the     or        r  t,prresses  r  d  gang  memb°'-s'  ™IMS 

the  course  of  a  study  o  Ch  icl  lPf  °ty  mks  "aVe  been  ^wtopcd  I. 
with  support  from  the  Si  f  ;  t°riier  groups'  undertakeiUl1  IW 
practically  finished  ""'  Ins"tute  of  Ment^  Health  and  iio« 


^  Dr;  J—  P.  Short,  J,,  and  Dr. 
sociologist,  is  now  dean  of  the  graduate 


*Wow  at  the  Department  of  <:„  •  i 
Washington.  P  ^^  °f  *"****.  Washington  State  University,  Pullman, 

384 


school  at  Washington  State  University,  but  during  the  study's  fieldwork 
served  at  the  University  of  Chicago  as  a  visiting  member  of  the  faculty.  Dr. 
Strodtbeck  holds  appointments  at  the  University  of  Chicago  in  the 
clepartments  of  sociology  and  psychology  and  is  director  of  the  Social 
Psychology  Laboratory.  Another  major  contributor  has  been  Dr.  Des- 
niond  S.  Cartwright,  now. at  the  Univeisity  of  Colorado. 

The  investigators  believe  their  study  to  be  the  largest  attempt  yet  made 
to  get  information  useful  in  understanding  and  dealing  with  teenage 
delinquent  gangs.  It  grew  out  of  a  new  piogram  under  which  college 
graduates  working  for  the  Y.M.C.A.  of  Metropolitan  Chicago  hang  around 
^vith  gang  membeis-listening  to  theii  talk,  going  along  with  them  to 
parties  and  to  encounters  with  other  gangs,  encouraging  them  to  take  up 
sports,  getting  them  admitted  to  ball  games,  seeing  that  they  are  treated 
for  venereal  disease,  talking  them  out  of  stolen  cars,  putting  them  in  touch 
"With  employment  programs,  and  so  on.  These  Y  employees,  assigned  one 
to  a  gang  and  known  as  detached  workers,  came  to  be  widely  accepted 
and  counted  upon. 

Through  weekly  interviews  with  the  detached  workers,  frequent 
observation  of  gang  activities,  talks  with  the  boys  themselves,  and  the 
administration  of  various  psychological  tests,  the  researchers  studied  16 
jgangs  in  considerable  detail.  These  gangs,  of  which  1 1  were  Negro  and  five 
White,  had  598  members. 

For  comparison,  the  researchers  studied  several  hundred  boys  who  lived 
in  the  same  areas  as  the  gang  members  but  did  not  belong  to  gangs.  They 
studied  also  two  Negro  and  two  white  groups  of  middle-class  boys,  all  of 
them  members  of  Y.M.C.A.  Hi-Y  clubs;  no  middle-class  gang  could  be 
located. 

The  principal  concern  was  with  group  motivations  and  processes— with 
what  makes  gangs  tick. 

The  project's  major  findings  are  summarized  in  the  numbered 
paragraphs  that  follow: 

1 .  Gang  members  and  other  lower-class  boys  express  as  high  a  regard  for 
middle-class  standards  as  do  middle-class  boys, 

This  finding  came  as  a  surprise,  Dr.  Strodtbeck  reports,  because  it  ran 
contrary  to  the  widely  accepted  theory  that  juvenile  gang  delinquency  is  a 
reaction  against  middle-class  standards. 

The  investigators  arrived  at  the  finding  by  using  a  semantic  differential 
to  study  values  held  by  the  boys.  A  semantic  differential  is  a  set  of  paired 
•words,  opposite  in  meaning,  for  learning  how  people  regard  a  given  set  of 
goals  or  standards.  Among  the  pairs  in  this  case  were  good-bad, 
pleasant-unpleasant,  and  smart-sucker.  Each  word  in  a  pair  was  one  end  of 
a  seven-point  scale.  If  a  boy  considered  something  wholly  good,  for 
example,  it  was  given  a  value  of  one;  wholly  bad,  a  value  of  seven;  just 
rnidway  between  good  and  bad,  a  value  of  four.  Among  the  images  rated 
by  the  scales  were  these: 

Representative  of  middle-class  standards; 

Someone  who  works  for  good  grades  at  school. 
Someone  who  likes  to  read  good  books. 

385 


someone  who  saves  his  money. 
Representative  of  lower-class  standards: 

Someone  who  has  a  steady  job  washing  and  greasing  cars. 
Someone  who  likes  to  spend  his  spare  time  hanging  on  the  come 

with  his  friends. 

Someone  who  shares  his  money  with  his  friends. 
Descriptive  of  many  boys  in  delinquent  gangs- 

Someone  who  is  a  good  fighter  with  a  tough  reputation. 
Someone  who  has  good  connections  to  avoid  trouble  with  Lhc  law. 
Someone  who  makes  easy  money   by   pimping  and  other  illegu 

hustles. 

All  boys  studied-gang,  nongang,  and  middle-class-evaluated  highly  the 
images  representing  salient  features  of  middle-class  style  of  life.  No  Oilier 
images  were  evaluated  more  highly. 

But  lower-class  images  did  get  a  higher  score  from  lower-class  boys  than 
from  middle-class  boys.  And  criminal  images,  though  rated  low,  received 
higher  scores  from  gang  membeis  than  from  nongang  members  and  higher 
scores  from  the  latter  than  from  middle-class  boys.  Further,  some 
evidence  was  found  that  gang  boys  more  than  others  might  be  inclined  to 
judge  participation  in  illegal  activities  from  the  standpoint  of  whether  or 
not  it  was  smart  rather  than  whether  or  not  it  was  good. 

The  boys'  ratings  of  themselves,  as  they  are  and  as  they  would  like  to 
be,  showed  that  regardless  of  the  group  to  which  a  boy  belonged  he  would 
like  to  be,  on  the  average,  both  smarter  (meaning  less  of  a  sucker)  niul 

better. 

2.  None  of  the  gangs  studied  can  be  characterized  as  strictly  criminal  or 
as  being  part  of  a  criminal  subculture.  However,  illegal  activities  were 
characteristic  to  some  extent  of  all  the  gangs. 

The  activities  of  gang  members,  as  reported  by  the  detached  workers, 
were  combined  into  37  items  of  behavior.  Factor  analysis  then  disclosed 
the  presence  of  five  groups  of  closely  associated  activities,  some  of  the 
groups  being  more  characteristic  of  certain  gangs  than  of  others.  The  five 
groups,  or  factors  follow: 

(a) Conflict.  Main  items:  individual  and  group  fighting,  the  carrying 
of  concealed  weapons,  and  assault.  Though  robbery,  theft, 
becoming  a  public  nuisance,  and  statutory  rape  also  appear  in 
this  factor,  they  are  part  of  other  factors  as  well. 

(b)  Stable  corner  activities.  Characterized  by  sports,  social  activities, 
and  gambling.  No  serious  delinquent  item  is  prominent. 

(c)A  cluster  of  behaviors  difficult  to  sum  up  but  finally  called 
stable  sex  pattern.  Principal  items  include  sexual  intercourse, 
statutory  rape,  petting,  and  the  buying,  selling,  and  use  of 
alcohol.  Holding  a  job  is  fairly  prominent,  too-the  only  factor  in 
which  it  is  prominent. 

frfjCalled  retreatist  because  the  buying,  selling,  and  use  of  narcotics 
and  marihuana  ranked  highest  among  the  activities.  Only  one 
retreatist  gang  was  found. 

386 


(e) Authority  protest.  Principal  activities:  stealing  autos,  driving 
without  a  license,  becoming  a  public  nuisance,  drinking,  running 
away  from  home,  playing  truant,  joyriding. 

From  the  individual  scores,  the  investigators  deteumned  the  average 
score  of  each  gang  on  each  factor.  Gangs  varied  most  greatly  on  (a),  least 
on  (e), 

The  ranking  of  a  gang  on  any  one  factor  was  found  to  correlate 
positively  with  its  ranking  on  all  the  other  factors  except  (b).  The  highei  a 
gang's  scoie  on  the  conflict  factor*  for  instance,  the  higher  its  score  on  all 
the  others  except  the  one  built  around  sports  and  social  activities. 

The  six  gangs  found  to  engage  in  the  most  conflict  were  all  Negro. 
Three  of  the  four  gangs  that  engaged  in  the  least  conflict  were  also  Negro, 
White  gangs  ranked  significantly  higher  than  Negro  only  on  the  "authority 
protest"  factor.  The  gangs  ranking  highest  in  "authority  protest"  were 
those  engaging  least  in  sports  and  other  activities  of(b). 

3.  The  white  community  is  more  concerned  than  the  Negro  with  the 
excesses  of  its  young  people,  and  in  general  controls  its  young  people 
more  effectively.  Among  the  many  and  complex  reasons  are  the 
greater  economic  stability  of  white  communities  and  the  existence  of 
indigenous  institutions  of  established  leadership. 

From  a  statistical  study  of  the  relationships  among  various  types  of 
behavior,  the  investigators  find  that  Negro  gang  members  tend  to  make  no 
clear  distinction  between  delinquent  and  conventional  behavior.  The 
Negro  gang  boys  participated  in  such  conventionally  approved  activities  as 
doing  the  household  chores  and  taking  part  in  organized  sports;  they  also 
participated  in  such  conventionally  disapproved  activities  as  fighting,  illicit 
sexual  relations,  drug  use,  and  auto  theft.  In  contrast,  white  gang  boys 
were  less  inclined  to  participate  in  approved  activities  and  were  more 
openly  at  odds  with  the  adult  community,  particularly  concerning 
rowdyism,  drinking  (almost  universal),  drug  use  (rare,  compared  with  the 
situation  in  Negro  gangs),  and  sexual  delinquency.  In  shoit,  the 
Investigators  suggest,  delinquency  among  Negro  gang  members  is  part  of 
the  total  life  pattern,  and  the  community  appears  to  accept  it  as  such. 

Life  in  the  lower-class  Negro  areas  was  found  to  be  organized  around 
sxich  institutions  as  "quarter  parties"  (sometimes  called  "rent  parties"), 
pool  halls,  and  taverns  to  a  greater  extent  than  in  comparable  white  areas. 
One  Negro  poolroom  that  was  a  hangout  for  prostitutes  and  thieves  was 
also  a  hangout  for  gang  boys.  Among  the  persons  present  on  one  occasion 
was  a  man  who  had  just  escaped  from  the  police,  stilt  wearing  one 
handcuff.  He  was  being  kidded. 

In  the  white  areas,  the  investigators  report,  life  generally  revolved 
around  more  conventional  institutions-the  Catholic  Church  in  particular, 
ethnic  groups,  unions,  bowling  leagues,  other  organized  recreational 
patterns,  and  political  and  "improvement"  associations:  these  last 
organized  in  large  measure  to  keep  the  Negroes  out. 

Economically  and  socially,  "no  white  area  [was  found  to  be]  as 
disadvantaged  as  the  least  disadvantaged  Negro  area."  The  Negro  gangs 
came  from  neighborhoods  where  the  median  family  income  ranged  from 

387 


S3, 200  to  S5  100:  the  white  gangs  came  from  neighborhoods  when; 
ranged  between  S 6, 200  and  S6,500.  Unemployment  was  10.6  portent 
the  Negro  areas;  4.8  percent  in  the  white  areas,  In  the  Negro  areas,  * 
peicent  of  the  dwelling  units  had  more  than  one  person  per  room;  in  tl 
white  aieas,  14  percent. 

4.  Rating  themselves  on  friendliness,  cleanness,  smartness, 
helpfulness,  and  goodness,  white  gang  boys  produced 
higher  scores  than  Negro  gang  boys.    With   the  two 
groups,  though,  (he  situation  was  reversed. 

if  the  Negro  gang  boys  reaily  are  so  fundamentally  disadvantjigccl  as  tt 
scores  indicate,  the  investigators  believe  that  "a  major  modification  of  tl 
social  system"  will  be  needed  to  change  "this  negative  self-image,"  (I 
middle-class  Negro  boy  rates  himself  highly,  the  study  suggests,  pnssib. 
because  he  may  see  himself  at  the  top,  looking  down,  whereas  Hie  micHIl 
class  white  boy  may  still  see  steps  ahead. 

The  investigators  reached  their  findings  by  having  the  boys  in  the  sUitl 
compare  themselves  and  their  associates  with  other  neighborhood  hoys  u 
the  basis  of  42  adjectives.  The  ratings— -more,  the  same,  (ess— were  givo 
numerical  values  and  subjected  to  statistical  analysis.  By  various  check 
the  researchers  satisfied  themselves  that  in  making  the  ratings  Um  ho> 
were  actually  describing  themselves. 

Some  other  findings: 

Among  Negroes,  the  higher  the  socioeconomc  status  (ranked  11 
follows:  middle-class,  lower-class,  nongang,  gang),  the  highev  the  boy 
rated  themselves  on  masculinity,  Among  whites  the  opposite  was  Inn 
What  is  involved,  the  investigators  suggest,  is  a  cultural  difference  in  \\\ 
degree  to  which  being  manly  is  a  focal  concern. 

The  gang  members  who  saw  themselves  as  polite,  loyal,  helpful,  swan 
i-iean,  obedient,  and  /-^(bus-rather  than  troublesome,  mean,  tough,  am 
coo! -were  also  the  boys  who  reported  engaging  in  more  cleHmjucn 
activity  than  the  others.  Explanation?  Perhaps  the  boys  with  111- 
characteristics  expressed  by  those  adjectives  are  the  ones  who,  more  thin 
others,  hold  the  group  together,  the  investigators  suggest,  and  f 
are  thrown  mto  roles  and  situations  leading  to  delinquent  acts, 

5.  Gang  members  more  than  the  other  boys  studied  seem  to  su 


in  interpersonal  retoltafis. 


markedly  higher  than  their 

more  likely  a  boy  was  to  rate iSVTL ATT  ^  Sodal  Icvc!> 

uccter  tnan  his  associates, 

388 


Among  lower-class,  particularly  gang  boys,  the  investigators  infer,  there 
exists  an  underlying  dissatisfaction  with  their  peers.  Beneath  the  surface 
solidarity  of  the  gang,  there  are  signs  of  mutual  frustration  and  lack  of 
trust.  Mental  health  depends  strongly  on  accepting  oneself,  and  the  ability 
to  accept  oneself,  the  investigatois  note,  seems  strongly  related  to  the 
ability  to  accept  others. 

The  inference  about  gang  boys  is  apparently  bolstered  by  a  personality 
assessment,  which  suggests  that  these  boys  are  less  self-assertive  than  the 
other  boys  studied,  more  reactive  to  false  signals,  slightly  more  neurotic 
and  anxious,  less  gregarious,  and  more  narcissistic.  In  sum,  they  are 
lacking  in  qualities  that  make  for  helpful  relations  with  others, 

As  an  example  of  gang  boys'  insecurity,  the  study  notes  that  they  were 
much  more  sensitive  than  nongang  boys  to  how  others  were  answering 
questions  and  performing  various  tasks;  they  were  more  anxious  about 
their  own  performance  as  compared  with  others.  Yet  gang  experience, 
with  its  constant  challenge  to  prove  such  qualities  as  toughness,  smartness, 
and  adeptness  with  the  girls,  probably  does  little  to  alleviate  such 
insecurity  except  in  the  gang  itself. 

6,  The  roots  of  social  disability  appear  to  lie  in  early  family  life. 

In  considering  the  causes  of  delinquency,  sociologists  and  psychologists 
often  emphasize  status  deprivation.  But  this  is  conceived  of  mainly  in 
occupational  and  economic  terms,  the  present  investigators  point  out, 
whereas  feelings  of  self-worth  determined  by  factors  not  directly 
connected  with  job  levels  and  income  rates  may  prove  equally  relevant. 

Observations  at  an  experimental  nursery  school  for  lower-class  Negro 
children-conducted  at  the  University  of  Chicago  by  Dr.  Stiodtbeck  under 
an  Office  of  Education  grant-suggest  that  at  the  age  of  41/2  years  these 
children  are  less  able  than  those  of  middle-class  homes  to  associate  with 
other  children  without  fighting.  They  seem  to  have  been  harshly  brought 
up  and  to  have  been  cautioned  frequently  that  the  world  around  them  is 
threatening.  Further,  they  are  retarded  in  cognitive  development  and 
verbal  skills,  partly  because,  the  grantee  finds,  they  have  lacked 
stimulation  from  reading  materials  and  constructive  play  and  partly 
because  they  have  not  been  exposed  to  an  atmospheie  that  rewards  good 
use  of  language. 

Gang  boys,  the  researchers  have  observed,  are  relatively  ignorant  of  how 
to  dress  for  a  given  occasion,  eat  in  public,  or  carry  on  a  polite 
conversation.  Their  limited  social  skills  are  attributed  to  their  narrow 
social  experience  not  only  within  the  gang  but  also,  and  first,  within  the 
family. 

The  family  does  not  equip  the  child  to  meet  the  demands  of  school,  the 
investigators  theorize,  and  unsatisfactory  experiences  in  school  further 
narrow  his  opportunities  to  play  roles  that  will  prepare  him  for  getting 
along  with  employers  and  fellow  workers  and,  in  general,  for  dealing  with 
new  situations.  In  contrast,  middle-class  parents  early  help  their  children 
develop  the  ability  to  adjust  as  the  situation  requires.  "Company"  is 
different  from  "family,"  and  entertaining  the  boss  teaches  approved 

389 


means  of  relating  to  authority  as  well  as  something  about  the  require 
merits  of  dress  and  manners. 

The  study  emphasizes  that  since  the  family  life  of  the  boys  WHS  no 
investigated,  there  is  as  yet  no  proof  of  the  suspected  differoncc* 
Neveitheless,  the  lower-class  nongang  boys  did  give  evidence  of  having  hai 
more  opportunities  to  learn  how  to  get  along  with  people,  Roughly  7'. 
percent  of  them  had  successfully  adjusted  to  school,  as  compared  witl 
about  50  percent  of  the  gang  boys.  Also,  more  nongang  boys  than  ganj 
boys  reported  having  had  contact  with  adults  of  relatively  high  status 
such  as  the  administrators  of  youth  agencies,  clergymen,  and  employers 

As  further  evidence  that  the  lower-class  nongang  boys  had  stiongc 
family  or  institutional  ties,  these  boys  spent  less  time  in  places,  inchidtni 
street  corners  and  poolrooms,  where  there  was  a  high  risk  of  becomiui 
involved  in  delinquency. 

Differences  in  intelligence,  too,  are  reported  as  contributing  to  Uu 
social  disability  of  gang  boys.  The  following  table  shows  1Q  estimates 
based  on  scores  on  so-called  "culture-free"  tests-tests  that  try  tc 
minimize  the  effects  of  previous  tiaining.  As  the  investigators  put  it,  UK 
school  and  other  social  institutions  reward  the  bright  boy-arid  I  he  #m£ 
boys  are  handicapped  in  respect  to  brightness. 


1Q  Estimates 

Negroes  .    . 

Gang 
69 

Nongang 
74 

Middle  class 
Ofi  S 

Whites  .  . 

85 

91.5 

JXl  *  J 

in 

7.  One  factor  in  the  formation  of  Chicago  's  juvenile  gangs  Is  Die-  It'ii.w, 
fear/ul  atmosphere  of  lower-class  areas 


^'°,IiVe  '"."V3™  area  v"y  long,  Dr.  Strodtbeck  comment*, 
Ru'  H  ?      fd?  t0vvard  one  another  "'at  >^pccl   properly 
"le  tt  »,  r  "iT"  l'^'1  Negr°  fam'Iy  has  moved  th"«  tillies 

" 


.V  e-  history'"  the  Srantee  continues,  «h.« 


no 

«  d  ,,     yfbeeir°re  distinctly  Jerked 
ft  ™f  ^        nfuP  "elWOrks'  Here  in  Wc^o,  Negroos  live  in 

™  H          fei  Jnoto^T'  Ma^a  tenement  door  has  lhreo  lwlts 
trm  We  cornel    l°m    ^  that  a  neiSh^  may  mean  Harm  but  also  Hint  if 

n7r  tr4e"  ™  --y°U  ^i-311  ^  '°  Wm>  Y°U  Wi"  bC  SUCktt' 
ways  of  X-ee™gpeoT  a  San  eTd  ^  Can"0t  be  shsred'  y°"  fi>K' 
•pu-tment  door  It  does  .in^^?™"1  communlty  sto"s  nt  »c 
extended  kin."  take  m  the  buildi"g.  «°r  the  block,  nor 


n    te 
too,  if  he  can.  S  °  aSSUme  that  every  «»»«  else  will  got  his, 


agency  representatives"  the  studv  r"     7  St'f  '°rs'  police'  and  other 


r 

which  other  potentiany  legitirna     s±  ™f  '     "^  ™  atmosP|lere   "" 

Numate  sources  of  support,  as  well  as  neighbors, 


390 


defined  as  part  of  a  hostile  outgroup.  The  result  is  that,  for  the 
a  liaison  which  is  as  informal  as  standing  on  the  comer  with 
boys  comes  to  be  sought  particularly  because  it  requires  so  little 
commitment  of  exposure." 

By  engaging  in  group  delinquency,  gang  members  help  to  satisfy 
common  adolescent  needs  both  for  working  with  and  depending 
upon  others  and  for  achieving  status. 

are  likely  to  bring' to  the  gang  a  suspicion  of  the  dependability  of 
relationships,  the  study  notes,  and  the  gang  contributes  to  this 
Status  within  the  gang  is  subject  to  constant  challenge,  and 
to  status  may  disrupt  even  close  friendships.  But  the  gang  does 
a  status  system  in  which  boys  can  succeed  after  they  have  failed  in 
~*Qre  conventional  settings. 

Because  adolescence  is  a  period  of  emancipation  from  childhood,  most 
find  it  difficult  to  admit  and  to  gratify  their  dependency  needs, 
is  particularly  the  case  among  gang  members,  who  are  undei  pressure, 
n  a  culture  that  values  toughness,  to  shy  from  anything  likely  to  be 
interpreted  as  a  sign  of  personal  weakness.  But  planning  and  canying  out 
^ang  forays  and  talking  about  them  afterwards— with  opportunities  to 
Ldrnit  to  fright  and  to  express  appreciation  for  help—create  a  strong 
Hough  temporary  bond  of  loyalty,  the  study  reports,  that  probably  serves 
o  meet  dependency  needs  for  a  time. 

The  investigators  point  out  that  a  gang  boy's  early  decisions  to  do 
something  illegal  are  not  necessarily  a  deliberate  attempt  to  develop  bonds 
tvith  other  boys:  Easy  material  and  aggressive  exploits  are  attractive  in 
tHemselves.  The  satisfaction  that  comes  from  working  with  a  group  may 
be  at  the  start  only  a  bonus. 

9.  Short-run  hedonism,  or  a  search  for  pleasure  without  regard  for  the 
consequences,  has  often  been  advanced  as  an  explanation  of  why 
gang  members  participate  in  seriously  delinquent  activities.  A  more 
valid  explanation  may  be  a  process  that  balances  the  immediate 
outcome  of  an  action,  in  terms  of  the  gain  in  status  if  one  joins  the 
action  and  the  loss  if  one  holds  back,  against  the  remote  possibility 
of  punishment  by  the  larger  society  if  something  goes  seriously 
wrong. 

In  putting  forth  this  theory  of  aleatory  risks,  or  risks  strongly  affected 
t>y  a  probability  process,  the  investigators  conclude  that  the  disposition  of 
gang  boys  to  join  the  action  cannot  be  explained  either  by  the  values  they 
Inold  or  by  neurotic  Or  irrational  tendencies.  Further,  the  researchers 
argue,  short-run  hedonism  is  not  a  good  explanation  because  the  term 
implies,  wrongly,  that  an  atmosphere  of  abandon  surrounds  gang  decisions 
and  activities. 

As  Drs.  Strodtbeck  and  Short  see  it,  in  such  a  loosely  organized  group 
as  a  juvenile  gang  there  tends  to  be  a  continuous  leakage  of  status  and, 
tlierefore,  a  need  for  continuously  working  status-maintaining 
mechanisms.  The  boy  who  participates  in  a  delinquent  action  wins  a  slight 
reward;  his  status  is  maintained  or  even  heightened. 

391 


To  be  sure-particularly  in  the  case  of  fights,  since  guns  and  knives  are 
common  in  lower-class  neighborhoods— there  is  a  chance  that  the  action 
will  lead  to  seiious  consequences,  with  some  of  the  participants  being 
hurt,  killed,  or  arrested.  But  the  investigators  estimate  that  not  more  than 
a  fifth  of  the  cases  do  have  serious  consequences  to  gang  members,  and 
they  point  out  that  not  more  than  a  fifth  of  the  offenses  actually 
committed  by  gangs  studied  were  known  to  the  police.  Hence  the  risks  are 
likely  to  appear  well  worth  taking.  A  boy  can  avoid  all  risk  by  staying 
aloof,  but  in  that  case  he  suffers  a  loss  of  status. 

1 0.  Members  of  delinquent  gangs  do  not  pursue  the  middle-class  goals 
they  espouse  because,  in  part,  (a)  they  don't  know  how  and  (b) 
gang  life,  which  they  value,  emphasizes  behavior  incompatible 
with  such  pursuit. 

Gang  boys  can  be  told  how  to  reach  middle-class  standards,  but  the 
information  rarely  penetrates  to  the  point  where  it  arouses  action.  In 
this  respect,  Dr.  Strodtbeck  adds,  they  are  like  an  inveterate  cigarette 
smoker  who  is  told  how  he  can  improve  his  health. 

The  investigators  believe  that  the  boys  mean  what  they  say,  both  alone 
and  in  groups,  about  such  values  as  getting  a  job,  saving  money,  getting 
married.  Alone,  the  boys  late  these  values  highly;  in  groups,  they  deride 
them.  In  this  respect  they  are  like  the  medical  school  students  who,  in 
another  study,  were  found  to  speak  idealistically  about  their  chosen 
profession  only  when  alone  with  the  investigator.  Contradictory  value 
systems,  the  study  notes,  are  a  mark  of  modern  society,  particularly 
among  adolescents. 

Gang  life,  as  well  as  medical  school,  may  be  seen  as  a  phase  in  a  person's 
career.  The  study  explains  that  no  gang  boy  expects  this  phase  to  last 
forever.  The  trouble  is  that  involvement  in  gang  life  hampers  the 
achievement  of  values  held  individually  with  respect  to  future  phases. 

Sex  is  one  example.  Among  gang  boys,  as  among  boys  generally,  it  was 
found  to  be  a  matter  of  much  concern  and  some  anxiety.  The  pressure  of 
the  gang  compounds  the  problem,  the  investigators  explain,  because  it  is 
harder  for  gang  than  for  nongang  boys  to  withdraw  from  sexual 
competition  and  excel  in  some  other  endeavor.  Among  the  gangs  studied* 
marriage  was  by  no  means  taboo  but  "making  out"  was  more  highly 
valued,  and  the  boys  who  were  the  gang  leaders  always  had  finesse  in 
sexual  matters. 

Becoming  the  parent  of  an  illegitimate  child  apparently  does  not  affect 
a  boy's  status  one  way  or  the  other,  and  it  may  actually  raise  a  girl's 
status.  Citing  other  studies,  the  investigators  observe  that  the  unmarried 
lower-class  Negro  girl  who  becomes  pregnant  may  suppose  she  has  some 
chance  of  landing  the  boy  as  a  husband;  in  any  event,  she  gets  gratifying 
attention  from  her  female  relatives  and  the  social  worker.  And,  Dr. 
Strodtbeck  adds,  the  financial  help  she  receives  from  the  Aid  to 
Dependent  Children  program  may  enable  her  to  pull  out  and  live  in 
slightly  more  savory  surroundings  that  those  at  home. 

"Both  boys  and  girls,"  the  report  concludes,  "are  caught  in  a  cycle  of 
limited  social  abilities  and  other  skills.  Their  disabilities  contribute  to  their 

392 


worries   about   status    and   in   this  way   contribute   to   involvement   in 
delinquency." 

As  with  sex,  so  with  work.  Individually  the  boys  begged  the  detached 
workers  to  help  them  get  jobs,  but  on  the  street  they  bragged  about 
"hustles"  of  great  variety.  During  one  3-year  period,  every  member  of  one 
of  the  gangs  held  at  least  one  job,  for  a  time,  at  the  end  of  the  period,  not 
one  of  the  members  was  employed.  The  investigatois  explain  that  the  kind 
of  work  such  boys  can  get  is  generally  neither  sufficiently  well  paid  nor 
sufficiently  challenging  to  serve  as  an  acceptable  status  alternative  to  the 
gang.  Further,  participation  in  gang  activities  is  not  conducive  to  a  good 
work  record,  and  a  good  work  record  is  not  conducive  to  participation  in 
gang  activities. 

One  gang  member  did  fall  in  love  with  his  job  as  messenger-in  part, 
because  it  gave  him  a  uniform  and  a  chance  to  talk  to  people.  He  married 
his  girl  and  began  buying  furniture  and  a  car.  Eventually  he  was  promoted, 
but  the  extra  money  was  not  enough  to  meet  the  credit  payments.  In 
addition,  the  new  job  kept  him  from  circulating  and  threatened  to  expose 
his  limited  reading  ability.  He  dropped  out  of  legitimate  work  and  is  now 
a  well-paid  employee  of  a  group  in  the  numbers  racket. 

1 1.  Gang  life  offers  few  if  any  constructive  tasks  because,  in  part  (a) 
the  leaders  must  choose  activities  at  which  most  of  the  members 
are  proficient;  (b)  boys  who  have  found  fellowship  and  other  re- 
wards in  delinquent  activities  may  find  the  level  of  reward  in 
conventional  activities  too  low;  (c)  the  leaders  know  that  the 
easiest  way  to  meet  a  threat  to  their  status  is  to  engage  in  de- 
linquent activity. 

Team  sports  might  seem  a  promising  basis  for  group  solidarity,  the 
study  notes,  but  gang  boys  tend  to  be  aggressively  poor  losers.  The 
Y.M.C.A.  program  attempts  to  get  around  this  handicap  by  seeing  that 
every  team  in  a  tournament  gets  a  trophy —for  participation  or  sportsman- 
ship, if  not  for  winning.  The  program  also  appoints  influential  gang  mem- 
bers as  "field  consultants*'  at  a  small  salary.  Hence,  at  sports  events, 
consultants  from  rival  gangs  know  one  another  and  feel  bound  to  some 
degree  to  try  to  keep  peace.  Without  such  devices,  the  study  reports,  team 
contests  would  often  erupt  into  gang  warfare. 

V 

12.  The  fewer  the  opportunities  to  which  a  group  is  exposed  in  its 
own  area— for  example,  recreational  centers,  counseling  agencies, 
churches,   schools,    chances  to  work— the  more  the  group  is  in 
trouble  with  the  law, 

Based  on  interviews  concerning  "the  area  where  your  group  hangs  out," 
the  study  developed  opportunity  scores  for  each  boy  studied  and  for  each 
group.  These  scores— which  reflected  the  boys'  awareness  as  well  as  the 
actual  existence  of  the  opportunities-were  then  compared  with  the 

393 


average  number  of  offenses,  per  group  member,  known  to  the  police.  Tiie 
table  gives  the  results. 

Opportunity  Scores  versus  Delinquent  Behavior 

Offenses,  per 

Oppoitumty        boy,  known  to 
Group  score  police 


Negro  gangs  

9.0 

3.14 

White  gangs  

9.3 

2.73 

Negro  lower  class  ,    

11.0 

.47 

White  tower  class  

13.7 

.31 

Negro  middle  class  

15.6 

.06 

White  middle  class  

20.2 

.02 

Possibly  the  most  powerful  influence  against  delinquency,  Dr. 
Strodtbeck  reports,  is  the  opportunity  for  a  youngster  to  step  into  a  job  as 
soon  as  he  leaves  school.  "In  Europe,"  he  observes,  "when  you  get  out  of 
school,  at  any  level,  you  can  get  a  job  that  pays  in  accordance  with  the 
contribution  you  can  make  to  the  production  process.  You  have  more 
trouble  doing  that  here— and  more  delinquency." 


For  the  Future 

The  detached  worker  program,  Di.  Strodtbeck  reports,  has  been 
successfully  used  to  prevent  gang  fights  and  is  now  being  experimented 
with  as  a  gateway  to  what  sociologists  call  "the  opportunity  structure,"  If  a 
boy  can  become  interested  in  employment,  perhaps  he  can  also  become 
interested  in  going  back  to  school  or  in  getting  more  education  and 
training  some  other  way.  And  if  the  road  can  be  cleared  to  steady 
employment  for  one  boy,  perhaps  he  can  become  a  channel  through 
which  other  boys  can  be  reached.  But  there  are  many  forces  pressing  on  a 
gang  boy  to  remain  pessimistic  about  his  chances  in  the  larger  world. 

Research  Grant:  MH  3301 
Reference' 

Short,  James  F.,  Jr.,  and  Strodtbeck,  Fred  L.  Group  Process  and  Gang  Delinquency. 
University  of  Chicago  Press,  Chicago,  1 965. 


Investigators. 

Malcolm  W.  Klein,  Ph.  D. 

Helen  E.Shimota,  Ph.  D. 

University  of  Southern  California 

Los  Angeles,  Calif. 

Prepared  by: 

Gay  Luce 

Juvenile  delinquents,  according  to  court  records,  account  for  less  than  a 
million  offenders  in  the  country.  But  their  impact  ramifies,  and  the  social 
costs  of  their  destructive  behavior  cannot  be  evaluated  by  a  head  count.  A 
high  rate  of  illegitimacy  and  a  new  generation  of  underprivileged  children 
are  among  these  costs.  Because  roughly  four-fifths  of  all  juvenile  arrests 
are  boys,  and  because  their  gang  activities  have  been  most  conspicuous  and 
amenable  to  study,  the  preponderance  of  our  information  about 
delinquent  gang  behavior  and  background  almost  omits  the  part  played  by 
girls,  an  information  gap  that  should  be  filled  if  our  remedial  action 
programs  are  to  take  root. 

What  is  the  nature  of  the  delinquent  girl  gang?  How  does  it  influence 
the  boys'  gang?  Who  joins,  and  how  does  she  differ  from  her  nongang 
counterparts? 

Some  answers  to  these  questions  have  emerged  from  a  study  in  south 
central  Los  Angeles.  The  neighborhood,  75  percent  Negro,  was  a 
residential  region  covering  many  square  miles.  Incomes  ranged  from  under 
$1,000  a  year  to  $10— $15,000.  Unlike  the  congested  eastern  slum,  this 
was  an  environment  of  small  houses,  duplexes,  and  apartments,  spaced  by 
grass  and  trees.  Less  than  2  percent  of  the  adolescent  population  there 
belonged  to  gangs,  and  the  gang  membership  was  often  dispersed  over  a 
square  mile.  In  the  course  of  an  extensive  study  of  gangs  and  delinquency, 
the  investigators  singled  out  a  number  of  boys'  gangs  with  a  membership 
totaling  about  600,  and  a  half  dozen  or  so  sister  gangs,  whose  membership 
amounted  to  about  1 50.  These  were  Negroes  ranging  in  age  from  1 2  to  25. 
Their  gangs  were  in  constant  metamorphosis,  shifting  in  membership, 
disbanding,  reforming.  A  rounded  picture  of  these  youngsters,  their 
backgrounds,  psychometric  evaluation,  their  influence  upon  one  another 
and  opinion  of  each  other  was  obtained  with  the  help  of  a  variety  of  Los 
Angeles  agencies,  the  schools,  the  police^  the  juvenile  court,  and 
particularly  with  the  aid  of  special  group  workers  assigned  to  the  gangs  by 
the  County  Probation  Department. 

The  procedure  of  collecting  information  utilized  these  sources  at  three 
levels.  In  order  to  search  out  the  interaction  and  influence  of  girl  and  boy 
gangs  upon  each  other  and  to  determine  the  nature  of  these  groups,  a 
system  of  personal  interviews  was  established  in  which  the  gang  members, 

395 


detained  for  a  delinquent  offense,  weie  asked  to  describe  the  role  they 
played,  the  roles  of  their  associates  and  of  the  girls,  and  to  give  their  own 
peiceptions  of  the  event.  These  were  compared  with  police  records,  and 
with  the  diiect  observations  of  group  workers  and  the  study  staff. 
Interestingly  enough,  the  youngsters  cooperated  well  and  offered  candid 
descriptions  that  jibe  with  the  reports  of  police  and  group  workers. 

The  half  dozen  or  so  girl  gangs  under  scrutiny  varied  considerably  in 
size,  cohesiveness,  and  activity.  One  of  the  larger  groups  had  never  met  as 
an  organized  unit.  Another  gang  concentrated  upon  making  life  miserable 
for  schoolmates  and  fighting  giils  who  were  not  gang  membeis.  The  most 
extreme  gioup  indulged  m  violence,  hitchhiking  and  mugging  drivers, 
robbery,  and  vandalism.  This  gang  dispersed  when  many  of  its  members 
became  piegnant,  and  was  succeeded  by  another  gang  of  20  younger  girls 
who  may  be  carrying  on  the  tradition. 

In  the  course  of  the  study  it  became  clear  that  gangs  of  girls  were  not  as 
long-lived  as  boys'  gangs.  They  were  indeed  dependent  units,  and  with  one 
exception,  they  all  began  in  relation  to  a  boys'  gang.  They  were 
characterized  by  greater  turnover  in  membership,  and  tended  to  disband  if 
their  brother  gang  dispersed. 

Within  the  literature  on  delinquency  it  has  been  speculated  that  sister 
gangs  often  play  the  devil's  advocate,  inciting  violence,  starting  fights, 
encouraging  greater  and  grander  illegal  plans.  The  findings  within  the 
Negro  gangs  in  south  cential  Los  Angeles  would  indicate  an  opposite 
effect,  By  questioning  boy-gang  members  in  this  area  after  each  incident, 
it  became  apparent  that  girls  rarely  participated  in  the  planning  or  action, 
Frequently,  the  girl's  role  was  that  of  an  observer  on  the  fringes  of  theft 
or  assault.  When  asked  how. a  girl's  presence  on  the  scene  would  influence 
plans  or  action,  almost  half  of  the  boys  replied  that  the  accidental 
presence  of  girls  would  have  postponed  or  prevented  the  event,  especially 
if  the  plan  were  a  theft.  In  good  measure,  the  boys'  responses  were  a 
reflection  of  a  need  for  esteem.  Among  the  adolescent  boys,  notably,  the 
girls'  opinion  weighed  heavily,  and  few  of  the  boys  thought  they  would 
win  respect  for  attempting  an  offense  and  getting  caught.  In  sum,  the  boy 
who  valued  the  girls'  opinion  of  him  said  he  would  be  prevented  or 
delayed  in  his  illegal  plan,  It  seemed  that  the  boys  could  take  chances  of 
being  apprehended  without  worrying  about  their  image,  but  if  a  girl 
actually  witnessed  the  act  there  would  be  no  way  of  denying  or 
disclaiming  the  behavior. 

Although  the  actual  pillage  and  violence  perpetrated  by  these  juvenile 
gangs  is  probably  overdramatized,  these  are  youngsters  who  disrupt  their 
neighborhoods  and  schools,  use  drugs,  and  destroy  more  than  they  con- 
struct. They  are  part  of  a  minority  group  in  an  extended  ghetto,  low  in 
income  and  high  in  unemployment.  They  are  a  small  part  of  a  major  social 
problem  under  study,  a  group  whom  remedial  classes  and  camps,  and 
special  resocializing  schools  can  barely  hope  to  help  utilize  their  potential. 
Perhaps  the  major  burden  of  their  delinquent  pattern  is  borne  by  the 
generation  beyond  their  own,  for  the  social  cost  of  their  many  illegitimate 
children  outweighs  the  robberies  and  assaults  they  conduct  today.  An 

396 


understanding  of  female  gangs  may  indeed  make  it  possible  to  counteract 
this  trend. 

Research  Grant:  MH  7993 

Date  of  Interview  February  25,  1965 


397 


Investigator' 

Barbara  Fish,  M.D. 

Mew  York  University  Medical  Center 

New  York,  N.Y. 

Prepared  by: 
Herbert  Yahraes 


An  improved  method  of  evaluating  the  effect  of  psychiatiic  drugs  on 
emotionally  disturbed  children  has  been  developed  by  the  Childien's 
Psychopharmacology  Unit  of  the  New  York  University  School  of 
Medicine,  Department  of  Psychiatry,  and  is  being  applied  to  the  study  of  a 
number  of  compounds. 

The  Unit,  established  in  1961  with  the  aid  of  a  special  grant  from  the 
National  Institute  of  Mental  Health,  is  directed  by  Dr.  Barbara  Fish, 
psychiatrist-in-charge  of  the  Childien's  Service  of  Bellevuc  Hospital's 
psychiatric  division, 

Dr.  Fish  and  her  fellow  workers  have  confirmed  that  a  child's  response 
to  a  psychiatric  drug  may  be  quite  different  from  an  adult's.  For  example, 
triflu opera zine  (Stelazine),  a  tranquilizer  having  less  sedative  action  than 
chloipromazine,  was  found  to  be  a  stimulating  agent  when  given  to 
retarded  schizophrenic  children  and  to  be  capable,  on  the  basis  of 
preliminary  findings,  of  bringing  moderate  improvement.  The  hope  is  that 
even  more  potent  agents  will  be  discovered. 

To  get  a  trustworthy  measure  of  a  drug's  effect,  the  Unit  has  found  it 
necessary  to  divide  emotionally  disturbed  children  into  four  general 
groups  and  to  observe  the  results  of  medication  in  each  group.  These 
groups  or  types,  constituting  a  clinical  typology,  are  as  follows: 

I.  Autistic-dysjunctive:  severely  impahed  schizophrenic  children. 

II.  Immature-labile:    a   borderline   group,   less   severely    impaired   til 
present  than  the  children  of  group  I  but  more  severely  impaired  at  present 
than  the  children  in  groups  III  and  IV. 

III.  Anxious-neurotic:  children  with  picdominantly  neurotic  manifesta- 
tions. These  children  show  anxiety  and  feelings  of  helpless  dependence 
and  inadequacy. 

IV.  Sociopathic-paranoid:  children  with  predominantly  sociopathic  or 
paranoid   features.  These  children  tend  to  deny  personal  responsibility 
for  their  feelings  and  acts;  their  behavior  may  be  antisocial;  they  may  be 
overly  suspicious. 

The  children  in  the  first  two  groups  are  the  most  severely  disturbed  and 
those  in  the  last  two,  the  least  disturbed.  The  classification  is  done  on  the 
basis  of  psychiatric  interviews.  Specially  developed  rating  scales  are  used 

398 


to  determine  the  severity  of  each  child's  condition  before  and  after  treat- 
ment. 

In  one  pilot  study  of  childien  on  the  psychiatric  ward  of  Bellevue  Hos- 
pital, the  Unit  has  investigated  the  effect  of  chlorpromazinc  and 
diphenhydramme  on  each  of  the  four  groups.  Chlorpiomazine  (thorazine) 
is  a  widely  used  tranquilizer;  diphenhydramine  (benadryl)  is  an 
antihistamine  that  makes  some  adults  sleepy  but  acts  in  young  children  as 
a  mild  tranquilizer. 

Of  the  more  severly  disturbed  children—those  in  groups  I  and  II—  80 
percent  improved  on  chlorpromazine  and  50  percent  on  diphen- 
hydramine. None  improved  when  given  only  a  placebo. 

Howevei,  among  the  less  seveiely  impaired  children— those  in  groups  III 
and  IV— the  percentage  of  those  who  impioved  on  chlorpiomazine  did  not 
differ  significantly  from  the  percentage  of  those  who  improved  though 
they  received  nothing  but  a  placebo.  The  improvement  in  several  of  the 
chlorpromazine-treated  children,  however,  was  more  marked.  This 
indicates  that  a  large  proportion  of  the  children  in  groups  III  and  IV  get 
better  or  worse  for  reasons  having  nothing  to  do  with  drug  action. 

To  sum  up,  improvement  in  the  fiist  two  groups  of  children  seems  to 
depend  primarily  upon  the  effectiveness  of  the  drug  being  studied  and  in 
the  second  two  groups  upon  such  factors  as  hospitalization,  psycho- 
therapy, and  special  education.  If  the  children  in  this  study  had  not  been 
classified,  chlorpromazine  would  have  appeared  effective—that  is,  better 
than  placebo  treatment— for  the  entire  group.  In  reality  it  was  significantly 
better  only  among  the  severely  impaired  children. 

•|!     *     $    H       i! 

When  drugs  are  evaluated,  the  investigators  report,  children  must  be 
matched  by  age  as  well  as  by  severity  of  illness,  because  some  types  of 
treatment  are  more  effective  with  certain  age  groups  than  with  others.  For 
example,  all  the  children  who  improved  with  diphenhydramine  were 
under  81/2  years  of  age.  Older  children  were  not  helped  by  this  drug.  Again, 
all  those  children  who  improved  when  receiving  only  a  placebo  were  more 
than  ten  yeais  old.  On  the  othei  hand,  there  were  children  in  all  age 
groups  who  improved  with  chloipromazine. 

Dr.  Fish  points  that  whether  or  not  a  particular  kind  of  behavior  is 
abnormal  often  depends  on  the  age  of  the  child,  since  behavior  ap- 
propriate to  one  age  may  be  immature  or  markedly  deviate  if  it  occurs  in 
an  older  child.  "The  simple  fact  that  the  child  is  a  growing  organism,"  she 
observes,  "complicates  every  aspect  of  the  evaluation  of  psychiatric  treat- 
ment. .  . .  One  must  detect  changes  produced  by  therapy  in  an  organism 
that  is  already  in  the  process  of  change." 

In  a  second  pilot  study,  the  Children's  Psychopharmacology  Unit  is 
investigating  the  effects  of  trifluoperazine  on  a  number  of  group  I  children 
raning  in  age  from  2  to  6.  Trifluoperazine  is  a  tranquilizer  used  for  chronic 
adult  patients.  Dr.  Fish  found  several  years  ago  that  it  increased  the  alert- 
ness and  motor  drive— and  sometimes  even  the  responsiveness  and 
language  ability-of  severely  apathetic,  withdrawn,  schizophrenic  children. 

399 


The  children  selected  for  the  current  study  showed  gross  withdrawal 
and  greatly  impaired  speech.  With  each  child  the  dose  of  trifluoperazine 
was  gradually  built  up  to  the  level  at  which  the  drug  was  doing  the  most 
good  without  causing  side  effects,  With  some  children  the  best  dose  was 
six  times  as  large  as  with  others.  Dr.  Fish's  team  also  noticed  considerable 
differences  among  individuals  in  the  doses  of  chlorpromazine  and  diphcn- 
hydramine  required  for  best  results. 

So  far  only  12  children  have  been  followed  in  the  trifluoperazine  study, 
Four  of  these  have  improved  on  the  drug  as  compared  with  one  in  the 
control  group.  They  show  increased  alertness  and  improved  language  and 
social  ratings.  These  results,  however,  are  not  yet  statistically  significanl. 

The  investigators  are  now- 

-drawing  more  children  into  the  study,  to  make  a  total  of  24; 

-trying  other  potentially  stimulating  drugs  on  the  childien  who  have 

not  improved  on  trifluoperazine; 

—developing  ways  to  measure  changes  in  alertness,  in  order  to  facilitate 

the  rapid  screening  of  compounds. 

Hi  *  t   *  $ 

Dr.  Fish  estimates  that  at  least  60  peicent  of  severely  disturbed  children 
can  be  moderately  improved  by  presently  available  drugs.  Among  children 
in  an  outpatient  population  who  were  too  distuibed  to  benefit  from 
psychotherapy,  drugs  enabled  one-fourth  to  go  to  regular  schools  and 
another  one-half  to  participate  in  group  activities  and  special  classes. 

Special  screening  of  drugs  potentially  valuable  for  disturbed  children  is 
essential,  the  New  York  University  Unit  reports,  because  a  drug's  action 
may  depend  upon  the  stage  of  the  child's  development.  This  is  true  in  the 
case  of  diphenhydiamine,  but  a  better  known  example  is  phenobarbilal, 
which  calms  adults  but  may  excite  young  children.  Quite  possibly,  drugs 
that  are  ineffective  in  adult  animals  or  human  beings  may  prove  valuable 
for  children. 

The  investigators  point  out  that  dosages  of  psychopharmacologic  drugs 
must  be  determined  separately  for  children  and  not  merely  adjusted  from 
adult  dosages  according  to  body  weight.  A  dosage  that  has  been  scaled 
down  in  this  manner  may  be  so  high  for  a  child  as  to  cause  toxic  symp- 
toms, or  ft  may  be  so  low  as  to  be  ineffective. 

Research  Grant    MH  4665 
Published  References 

Fish,  Barbara.  Drug  Therapy  in  Child  Therapy,  Psychological  Aspects.  Comprehensive 

Psychiatry,  1:1,  February  1960. 
Fish,  Barbara,  The  Influence  of  Maturation  and  Abnormal  Development  on  the 

Responses  of  Disturbed  Childien  to  Dntgs,  Reprinted  from  proceedings  of  Thntl 

World  Congress  of  Psychiatry,  June  1961. 
Fish,  Barbara.  Evaluation  of  Psychiatric  Therapies  in  Children.    Delivered  at  the 

meeting  of  the  American  Psychopathologicai  Association,  February  24,  1 962. 
Fish,  Barbara  Progress  Report  to  NIMH,  February  1963. 
Fish,  Barbara  and  Shapiro,  Theodore,  .4  Typology  of  Children's  Psychiatric  Disorders: 

Its  Application  to  a  Controlled  Evaluation  of  Treatment.  Presented  at  the  annual 

scientific  meeting  of  the  Academy  of  Child  Psychiatry,  September  1963. 

400 


Investigator. 

Leon  Eisenberg,  M.D.* 
Johns  Hopkins  University  School  of  Medicine 

Baltimore,  Md. 

Prepared  by. 

Herbert  Yahraes 

Significant  findings  in  the  ticatment  of  emotionally  disturbed  children 
are  reported  by  a  clinical  research  group  at  the  Johns  Hopkins  University 
School  of  Medicine.  The  most  important  result  of  the  work  to  date,  in  the 
view  of  the  group's  leader,  Dr.  Leon  Eisenberg,  professor  of  child 
psychiatry,  is  its  demonstration  that  effective  treatment  depends  upon 
accurate  diagnosis.  The  group  reports  that: 

1.  Brief    psychotherapy-half   a    dozen    sessions-worked   a   marked 
improvement  in  the  behavior  of  disturbed  children  diagnosed  as  neurotic. 

2.  Disturbed  children  diagnosed  as  hyperkinetic,  or  overactive,  showed 
little  response  to  psychotherapy  but  improved  to  a  significant  extent 
when  treated  with  a  stimulating  agent.  (Such  agents  have  been  used  for 
some  years  with  hyperkinetic  children;  the  Johns  Hopkins  work  verifies 
their  effectiveness  in  a  carefully  controlled  study.) 

3.  The   same   stimulating  agent  markedly  improved  the  behavior  of 
delinquent  boys  in  a  training  school. 

The  work  suggests  that  the  country's  lesources  for  helping  disturbed 
children  before  they  become  disturbed  adults  can  be  stretched  through 
the  wider  use  in  appropriate  cases  of  a  short-term  course  of  treatment 
instead  of  long-term,  intensive  psychotherapy. 

The  research  also  suggests  that  certain  psychoactive  drugs  may  be  useful 
in  the  management  of  institutionalized  delinquents.  There  is  at  least  the 
possibility  that  they  could  make  institutionalized  youngsters  more 
amenable  to  training  and  education  and,  therefore,  less  likely  to  return  to 
antisocial  activities. 

In  current  work  Dr.  Eisenberg  and  his  associates  are  trying  to:  (a)  Find 
out  how  psychoactive  drugs  work-that  is,  what  functions  they  affect;  (b) 
find  more  objective  ways  of  diagnosing  disturbed  children  and  measuring 
the  effects  of  therapy;  and  (c)  provide  more  information  on  some  of  the 
defects  in  mental  functioning  that  result  in  low  IQ  scores. 

*Now  at  the  Department  of  Psychiatry,  Massachusetts  General  Hospital,  Boston, 
Massachusetts 

401 


All  these  studies  should  lead  to  improvements  in  therapy  for  disturbed 
children.  The  investigators  hope  that  the  third  study  will  also  lead  to 
stionger  efforts  to  prevent  reasoning  disabilities  in  children  from  deprived 
homes.  The  iccently  completed  and  continuing  research  of  the  Hopkins 
group  is  presented  in  more  detail  in  the  following  sections. 

Neurotics  and  Hyperkmetics 

In  a  pioject  now  nearing  completion,  Dr.  Eisenberg  undertook  to 
expand  the  results  of  earlier  work  by  his  group  in  the  Children's 
Psychiatric  Clinic  of  the  Johns  Hopkins  Hospital.  This  had  shown  that 
disturbed  children  given  a  placebo  and  psychotherapy  for  7  weeks  were  at 
least  as  likely  to  register  maiked  improvement  as  disturbed  children  given 
a  tranquilizer  and  psychotherapy.  But  the  improvement  was  closely 
related  to  the  type  of  disoider.  Two-thirds  of  the  neurotic  child: en,  bill 
only  one-third  of  the  hyperkinetic  children,  showed  substantial  gains. 
Later  studies  indicated  that  most  hyperkinetics  did  respond  to  either  of 
two  stimulating  agents,  dextroamphetamine  and  methylphenidate. 

The  investigator  describes  the  neurotic  child  as  shy,  introverted,  afraid 
of  new  situations,  inhibited,  anxious.  Typically  the  parents  complain  that 
he  is  feaiful,  won't  go  out  and  make  friends,  can't  even  be  dragged  to 
parties.  The  hyperkinetic  child,  in  contrast,  is  distractible  and  forever  on 
the  go.  In  school  he  pays  attention  to  every  disturbance  rather  than  to  the 
main  activities  of  the  class.  He  does  not  follow  directions.  He  is  often 
accused  of  being  aggressive  because  he  doesn't  keep  his  hands  to  himself. 
His  parents  complain  that  they  cannot  manage  him  and  are  afraid  ho  is 
going  to  get  into  serious  trouble. 

The  new  project  called  foi  both  a  psychotherapy  study  with  neurotics 
and  a  psychopharmacology  study  with  hyperkinetics.  Eighty  children 
were  selected  for  each.  Ruled  out  were  children  whose  IQ  was  less  than 
80,  those  who  had  severe  neurological  defects,  those  who  were  in  trouble 
with  the  law,  or  those  who  were  so  sick  they  required  inslitutionalization. 
"I  don't  mean  you  can't  treat  such  patients,"  Dr.  Eisenberg  explains.  "I 
mean  they  were  different  from  the  ones  in  whom  we  were  interested  for 
this  particular  project.  We  wanted  to  define  our  population  carefully/1 

The  children  ranged  in  age  from  5  to  13  years.  On  the  basis  of  parent!)  1 
education,  they  came  from  three  social  classes-middle,  lower  middle,  and 
lower-but  mainly  fiom  the  first  and  second  of  these.  The  hyperkinetics 
tended  to  come  from  lower  social  levels  than  the  neurotics  and  to  be 
younger.  Most  of  the  children  had  been  referred  to  the  clinic  by  the 
schools  and  other  community  agencies;  some,  by  private  physicians.  In  a 
few  cases  mothers  had  called  the  clinic  themselves. 

The  Value  of  Brief  Psychotherapy 

The  problem  of  setting  up  a  control  group-untreated  persons  against 
whom  to  compare  the  treated  ones-for  the  psychotherapy  study  was 


particularly  difficult.  That's  because  the  process  of  including  a  person  in  a 
study  of  psychotherapy  may  in  itself  be  therapeutic.  "The  moment  you 
talk  to  a  disturbed  child  and  his  parents,"  Dr.  Eisenbcrg  points  out, 
"potentially  you  change  them— and  if  you  don't  talk  to  them,  you  don't 
know  what  the  child  was  like  at  the  beginning  of  the  study.  Mere 
questioning  may  amount  to  interpretation  even  when  the  doctor  has  not 
intended  to  give  advice.  For  example,  if  the  mother  says  that  the  child  is 
disobedient  and  you  then  ask  how  she  disciplines  him,  you  aie  implying  a 
connection  between  his  misbehavior  and  her  methods  of  discipline.  You're 
already  giving  treatment,  so  to  speak." 

The  control  gioup  in  this  case  was  a  consultation-only  group.  After  the 
intake  process— three  sessions  of  history  taking  and  psychological 
testing— the  parents  were  told  that  the  child  should  do  well  without  treat- 
ment if  certain  recommendations  were  followed.  The  recommendations 
were  tailored  to  the  case.  A  mother  who  was  exceptionally  harsh  or 
punitive  might  be  advised  to  let  up  on  the  discipline;  a  mother  who  was 
too  lenient  might  be  advised  to  show  more  firmness.  Parents  might  be 
given  suggestions  for  improving  relations  with  each  other. 

The  point  is  that  the  consultation  was  limited  to  one  30-minute  period, 
at  the  end  of  which  the  parents  were  assured  that  the  child's  condition 
would  be  checked  again  after  2  months.  (The  families  in  the  consultation 
group  were  offered  treatment  after  the  8-week  period  of  the  study  if  they 
wanted  it  and  the  doctors  thought  it  desirable.) 

In  contrast,  the  experimental  group  was  given  brief  psychotherapy, 
defined  as  five  additional  interviews  lasting  from  45  minutes  to  an  hour. 
During  these  periods  the  child  was  seen  by  a  psychiatrist,  and  one  or  both 
parents  by  a  social  worker. 

After  8  weeks,  most  of  the  children  had  improved,  but  those  in  the 
psychotherapy  group  significantly  more  so  than  the  others.  This  finding 
was  based  partly  on  ratings  by  the  psychiatrists  who  were  treating  the 
children,  and  such  ratings,  the  grantee  points  out,  may  be  unintentionally 
biased.  However,  the  psychiatrists'  ratings  were  confirmed  by  an  in- 
dependent set  of  ratings  made  by  the  children's  teacheis,  who  did  not 
know  to  which  group  a  child  had  been  assigned.  Further,  on  measures  of 
friendliness  and  aggresiveness,  derived  from  the  mothers*  descriptions  of 
the  children  on  a  rating  instrument  known  as  the  Clyde  Mood  Scale,  the 
childien  in  the  psychotherapy  group  showed  a  greater  change  for  the 
better  than  the  others. 

The  children  in  the  earlier  studies  who  showed  improvement  were 
found  a  year  later  to  be  maintaining  it.  The  children  in  the  new  study  will 
be  followed  up,  too. 

The  results  of  this  part  of  the  investigation  strengthen  Dr.  Eisenberg's 
conviction  that  psychiatric  clinics  and  psychiatrists  in  private  practice 
should  place  considerably  more  emphasis,  in  treating  disturbed  children  of 
the  neurotic  type,  on  brief  psychotherapy.  For  one  thing,  it  works.  For 
another,  a  given  clinic  or  individual  psychiatrist  can  reach  more  people 
with  it.  Further,  says  the  investigator,  "Brief  psychotherapy  makes  much 
more  sense  to  the  parents,  who  in  general  are  grateful  for  the  statement 

403 


that  you  will  see  their  child  so  and  so  many  times  instead  of  the  vague, 
'Well,  it  may  take  a  long  time.'  With  brief  psychotherapy,  there  are  fewer 
dropouts." 

The  results  of  this  form  of  treatment  have  not  been  compared  with 
those  of  long-term,  intensive  psychoteraphy,  the  investigator  observes. 
"But  if  I  were  the  director  of  a  community  agency,  with  the  task  of 
setting  up  a  program  for  disturbed  children  that  would  bring  the  greatest 
benefit  to  the  community  on  a  service  level,"  he  says,  "I  would  certainly 
put  much  moie  emphasis  on  brief  psychotherapy  than  has  been  tradition- 
al." 


Medicine  for  Hyperkinetics 

In  the  drug  study,  40  of  the  hyperkinetic  children  were  given  cither 
dextroamphetamine  or  methylphenidate,  commonly  used  with  adult 
patients  as  stimulants,  and  the  rest  were  given  a  placebo.  At  the  end  of  the 
8-week  study  period,  those  who  had  been  receiving  a  drug  were  rated  both 
by  clinic  personnel  and  by  teachers  as  significantly  more  improved  than 
the  others.  As  viewed  by  the  mothers,  they  still  scored  liigh  in  aggressive- 
ness but  less  than  before. 

On  several  objective  tests,  too,  the  drug-treated  group  surpassed  the 
other.  One  was  a  discrimination  task,  in  which  the  children  were  shown 
pairs  of  pictures  flashed  on  a  scieen  in  rapid  succession  and  asked  to  tell 
which  picture  in  each  set  was  the  larger.  When  the  time  between  the 
pictures  in  a  set  was  more  than  2  seconds,  no  change  occurred  in  either 
group  during  the  course  of  the  study.  But  at  a  spacing  of  2  seconds  the 
drug-treated  group  made  appreciably  fewer  errois  at  the  end  of  the  study 
while  the  other  group  showed  no  change. 

The  investigatois  consider  a  2-second  spacing  in  this  test  rather  stressful 
for  a  child.  So  the  drug  takes  hold,  they  think,  at  the  point  where  per- 
formance begins  to  break  down  under  pressure. 

Dr.  Eisenberg  was  especially  impressed  by  the  results  of  the  For  tens 
Maze  Test,  which  calls  for  the  subject  to  trace  his  way,  with  a  pencil, 
through  a  series  of  mazes  of  increasing  difficulty.  When  a  child  enters  a 
blind  alley,  he  is  scored  as  having  failed  on  that  particular  maze  even 

^i^i!16  baCkS  °Ut  and  continucs  in  the  right  direction.  An  impulsive 
child,  then,  is  likely  to  end  up  with  a  low  score.  Both  groups  of  hy- 
perkinetic children  made  poor  scores  at  the  beginning  of  the  study.  Eight 
«'^ks  later  the  placebo  group  showed  no  improvement,  but  the  scores  of 
Irug-treated  group  shot  up  about  15  points. 

t;"gly,  the  children  with  the  lowest  IQ's-all  of  which  were 
"-ma!  range-showed  the  greatest  improvement.  "Apparently 
is  functioning  well/'  Dr.  Eisenberg  explains,  "isn't  going  to 
the  drug  to  levels  of  superfunction.  But  children  with 
Q's  may  have  a  better  potential  than  the  IQ  scores  indicate. 
>e  using  it  because  of  their  inability  to  pay  attention  and  to 


control  their  motor  activity.  These  are  the  childicn  who  may  be  helped 
most,  at  least  in  the  skills  demanded  by  the  maze  test." 

This  reasoning  may  also  explain  some  of  the  results  of  earlier  work  with 
emotionally  disturbed  children  in  two  institutions.  In  each  case,  the  in- 
vestigator found  a  strong  trend  for  the  childien  who  had  been  given  a 
stimulating  drug  to  make  fewer  mistakes  than  the  others  in  learning  a 
standard  laboratory  test.  (The  children  in  this  preliminary  woik  were  not 
differentiated  either  by  the  type  of  disorder  or  by  IQ  scores.) 

To  test  the  idea  that  a  stimulating  agent  improves  the  learning  ability  of 
certain  disturbed  children  by  improving  their  ability  to  pay  attention,  Dr. 
Eisenberg's  associate.  Dr.  C.  Keith  Conners,  has  now  worked  out  a  way  of 
measuring  the  latter.  The  child  being  tested  watches  a  pattern  of  lights  on 
a  panel  and  presses  a  particular  light  whenever  a  new  pattern  appeals.  He 
has  to  keep  watching  the  panel  in  order  to  press  the  right  light  at  the  right 
time.  The  idea  is  to  learn  how  many  mistakes  he  will  make  over  a 
10-mimite  period.  Once  the  researchers  have  baseline  rates  for  disturbed 
children,  they  propose  to  test  a  variety  of  psychiatric  drugs  to  learn  if  the 
attention  span  can  be  favorably  altered.  If  the  plan  works  out,  the  group 
will  have  a  way  of  telling  beforehand  which  kind  of  drug  will  affect  a 
given  type  of  child  most  favorably. 

Why  should  a  stimulating  agent  work  with  hyperkinetics— children  who 
appear  to  be  already  overstimulated?  Before  answering,  Dr.  Eisenberg 
poses  a  companion  question:  Why  should  phenobarbital,  a  sedative,  tend 
to  overexcite  children  and,  often,  the  elderly?  The  usual  explanation,  he 
goes  on,  is  that  interfering  with  the  functions  of  certain  structures  of  the 
brain  may  produce  different  effects  at  different  stages  of  development. 
That  is,  a  given  drug  has  a  certain,  unchanging  pharmacologic  effect,  but 
the  physiologic  expression  of  this  effect  depends  on  the  balance  among 
the  brain  centcis,  and  this  balance  changes  as  the  brain  develops.  In  the 
case  of  stimulating  agents,  one  has  to  suppose  that  the  drug  in  some  way  is 
improving  the  inhibitory  or  control  centers  of  the  brains  of  hypeikinetic 
children.  If  scientists  can  learn  how  this  is  done,  the  search  for  more 
effective  drugs  will  be  greatly  advanced. 


Treating  Delinquents 

Twice  within  recent  years  the  Hopkins  team  has  gone  into  a  training 
school  and  tested  the  effect  of  psychoactive  drugs  on  delinquent  boys 
ranging  in  age  from  1 1  to  17.  The  first  time  the  team  used  perphenazine,  a 
tranquilizer.  Some  of  the  boys  in  the  study  were  given  the  tranquilizer, 
others  a  placebo,  and  the  rest  nothing.  Those  who  got  medicine,  whether 
it  was  the  active  drug  or  the  placebo,  responded  with  a  substantial 
improvement  in  behavior. 

"We  had  altered  the  social  environment,"  Dr.  Eisenberg  explains.  "All 
the  youngsteis  wanted  to  get  out,  and  the  medication  was  something  that 
was  supposed  to  make  them  better  and  help  them  get  out  sooner.  To  be  in 

405 


the  tieatment  cottage,  where  some  of  the  boys  were  on  perphenazine  and 
some  on  placebo,  became  a  matter  of  piestige." 

The  tranquilizer,  in  other  words,  had  a  placebo  effect:  the  boys 
expected  the  medicine  to  make  them  better  and  they  did  get  better.  In 
this  respect  they  were  just  like  the  boys  taking  the  placebo  itself.  Failure 
to  allow  for  such  an  effect  has  clouded  the  results  of  many  a  test  of  new 
medicines. 

When  the  team  returned  to  the  institution,  they  tested  dextro- 
amphetamine,  one  of  the  stimulating  agents  used  successfully  with  hy- 
perkinetics.  The  ding  was  given  to  some  of  the  boys  in  each  of  two 
cottages  and  this  time  the  results  were  quite  diffeient.  Only  the  boys 
receiving  the  medicine  showed  a  decided  improvement  in  behavior,  as 
rated  by  houseparents,  teachers,  and  cottage  mates.  However,  there  was 
some  improvement  among  their  associates,  both  those  who  were  untreated 
and  those  who  received  a  placebo,  and  this  change  may  have  resulted,  the 
investigators  suggest,  from  a  more  harmonious  atmosphere  brought  about 
by  the  change  in  the  drug-treated  boys. 

Though  the  improvement  lasted  no  longer  than  the  treatment,  the 
Johns  Hopkins  group  holds  that  further  research  with  delinquents  along 
the  line  it  has  pioneered  is  "a  compelling  social  necessity." 

"I  don't  think  you  can  change  the  symptoms  of  delinquency  merely  by 
medication,"  Dr.  Eisenberg  declares.  "But  if  you  can  diminish  a 
youngster's  anger  and  hostility  and  aggressiveness  so  that  instead  of  fight- 
ing everything  he  may  be  willing  to  listen  to  what's  said  to  him,  then  the 
ordinary  treatment  procedures  might  be  more  effective." 

Putting  it  another  way,  medication  may  lead  to  improved  behavior,  and 
improved  behavior  may  lead  to  bettei  relationships  with  houseparenls, 
teachers,  and  other  personnel  in  a  training  school,  and  these  better 
relationships  should  make  possible  a  more  constructive  outcome  of  a 
boy's  training  school  experience. 

So  far  as  Dr.  Eisenberg  knows,  this  lead  has  not  been  followed  up.  He 
points  out  that  most  of  the  facilities  for  delinquents  are  not  in  medical 
hands  and  do  not  have  much  medical  support.  Beyond  this,  when  staffs 
remain  underpaid  and  undertrained,  and  are  hard  put,  consequently,  to 
work  toward  the  professed  goal  of  reeducation,  medicine  that  might  be 
effective  in  other  circumstances  is  not  likely  to  do  much  good. 

The  investigator  doubts  that  drugs  can  help  the  delinquent  who  remains 
in  his  decaying  neighborhood  and  troubled  home.  Without  change  in  the 
social  circumstances  that  breed  delinquency,  he  says,  the  behavior  will 
almost  certainly  continue.  Social  rehabilitation  is  the  key  to  delinquency 
control;  drugs,  counseling,  and  other  measures  are  adjuncts  or  aids  that 
may  be  useful  and  essential-because  patterns  of  antisocial  behavior  tend 
to  persist— once  the  environment  has  been  altered. 

Tranquilizers 

The  use  of  stimulating  drugs  for  hyperkinetics  is  bound  to  increase  as 
the  lesult  of  the  recent  findings,  Dr.  Eisenberg  believes,  while  the  use  of 

406 


tranquilizing  drugs  for  disturbed  children  in  geneial  should  be  consider- 
ably reduced.  He  judges  that  the  tranquilizers  have  been  prescribed  far  too 
freely  in  the  treatment  of  children. 

No  drug  is  free  of  hazards,  he  holds,  and  no  child  should  be  placed  on 
medication  unless  there  is  a  clear  need  for  it  and  clear  evidence  that  the 
chosen  medicine  is  likely  to  be  beneficial.  None  of  his  group's  three  out- 
patient studies  with  disturbed  childten  produced  evidence  of  beneficial 
effects  that  could  be  ascribed  to  the  tranquilizers,  and  he  finds  little  or  no 
evidence  in  the  literature  that  tranquilizers  are  of  any  benefit  for  the  usual 
child  who  conies  into  an  outpatient  clinic.  With  the  schizophrenic  child 
the  story  is  different. 

Diagnosing  the  Disturbed  Child 

Because  of  considerable  overlap  in  types  of  psychiatrically  disturbed 
children,  the  John  Hopkins  group  is  looking  for  more  objective  diagnostic 
means.  Among  the  numerous  measures  being  studied  arc  the  answers  to  a 
symptom  questionnaire  filled  out  by  the  parents  of  the  400  disturbed 
children  treated  by  the  clinic  since  1959,  when  the  NIMH  began  support- 
ing its  research.  The  questionnaire  lists  70  symptoms,  or  types  of 
behavior,  and  the  parent  indicates  to  what  extent  each  of  these  applies  to 
his  child. 

Preliminary  analysis  shows  that  certain  clusters  of  these  symptoms  arc 
more  characteristic  of  one  type  of  disturbed  child  than  of  the  other. 
Neurotic  children  register  higher  on  the  clusters,  or  factors,  tentatively 
labeled  inhibited,  shy,  and  psychosomatic.  Hyperkinctics  register  higher 
on  the  factors  labeled  hyperactive,  tantrum  behavior,  aggressive  acting 
out,  and  sibling  rivalry.  The  symptoms  grouped  under  the  labels  anxious, 
stubborn,  and  school  problem  seem  to  apply  as  much  to  one  type  of  child 
as  to  the  other. 

In  work  now  going  on,  the  investigators  hope  to  leain  if  factor  scores 
provide  a  good  diagnostic  and  prognostic  tool  and  if  improvement  is 
related  to  changes  in  the  scores  on  particular  factors. 

Several  hundred  parents  of  normal  children,  approached  through  PTA's, 
have  filled  out  the  same  questionnaire.  The  researchers  wanted  to  learn  if 
such  parents  see  as  many  things  wrong  with  their  children  as  the  parents 
who  have  sought  help  from  the  clinic.  The  answer  is  that  they  don't: 
about  40  of  the  70  symptoms  were  found  to  have  been  checked  much 
more  frequently  for  the  psychiatric  population.  Through  factor  analysis 
the  reseachers  expect  to  come  up  with  a  detailed  description  of  how  a 
disturbed  child  differs  from  a  normal  child  in  terms  of  behavior  as  viewed 
by  the  parents. 

The  Thinking  Process  in  Children 

The  Johns  Hopkins  group  has  also  undertaken  basic  research  to  learn 
more  about  the  reasoning  processes  of  the  developing  child  and  thus  to  aid 

407 


in  understanding  and  treating  defects  in  these  processes.  As  Dr.  Eisenberg 
explains  it,  such  work  should  make  it  possible  in  many  cases  to  determine 
the  factors  responsible  for  a  low  IQ  scoie  and  do  something  about  them. 
This  phase  of  the  program  is  the  particular  interest  of  Dr.  Sonia  F.  Osier,  a 
psychologist. 

Dr.  Osier  began  by  testing  the  ability  of  normal  children  to  form  an 
idea  01  concept  and  by  studying  the  process  involved.  In  the  simplest  test, 
the  child  was  shown  two  pictures  at  a  time,  one  of  a  bird  and  one  of 
something  else.  The  fiist  pair,  for  example,  might  show  a  bluebird  and  an 
automobile;  the  second,  a  robin  and  a  dog;  the  third,  an  airplane  and  a 
canary.  There  was  a  lever  associated  with  each  picture,  and  the  child  had 
to  press  one  of  the  levers  each  time.  If  he  pressed  the  one  under  the  bird 
picture,  he  was  rewarded  with  a  marble.  Soonei  or  later,  most  childien  got 
the  idea  that  the  common  denominator  or  unifying  concept  was  birdam\ 
pressed  the  correct  levci  each  time. 

In  her  first  study,  Dr.  Osier  presented  three  concepts-bird,  animal,  and 
living  thing-to  groups  of  public  school  children  aged  6,  10,  and  14  years. 
Half  of  the  children  in  each  group  were  of  average  IQ  and  half  were  above 
average. 

Most  of  the  results  were  predictable.  The  bird  concept  proved  the 
easiest  to  grasp;  the  living  thing  concept,  the  hardest.  Older  children  did 
better  than  younger  children,  and  brighter  children  did  better  than  aveiage 
children.  Interestingly,  however,  the  average  and  the  bright  children  used 
different  approaches.  The  average  child  tended  to  follow  a  hit-and-miss 
process.  He  would  make  a  random  number  of  mistakes,  then  begin  getting 
the  right  answer  more  frequently,  and  finally  grasp  the  idea.  The  brighter 
children,  on  the  other  hand,  tended  to  go  along  making  mistakes  and  then 
jump  all  at  once  from  the  random  to  the  perfect  level  of  performance. 

The  results  suggested  that  while  the  average  child  was  proceeding 
blindly  on  a  trial-and-error  basis,  the  brighter  one  was  making  guesses  or 
hypotheses  and  at  some  point  saying,  "Ha!  Now  I  see-that's  what  the 
answer  is!" 

To  check  on  this,  Dr.  Osier  devised  a  test  calculated  to  confuse  the 
hypothesis  makers  by  providing  a  good  deal  of  irrelevant  information.  The 
problem  was  to  get  the  concept  of  two.  One  card  of  a  pair  might  show  a 
dog  and  a  ball;  the  other,  two  birds  and  an  automobile.  In  another  pair, 
one  card  might  show  a  shoe  and  a  baseball  bat;  the  other,  three  fire  trucks 
and  a  cat.  In  this  situation,  brightness  proved  no  asset.  The  higher  IQ 
children,  presumably  because  they  had  seen  and  been  obliged  to  discard  so 
many  hypotheses,  ended  up  scoring  no  better  than  the  others. 

Next  the  investigator  changed  the  rules  of  the  game  so  that  right 
answers  were  rewarded  only  one  time  out  of  two.  This  situation  bothered 
both  the  older  children  and  the  brighter  children  more  than  the  younger 
children  and  those  of  average  intelligence-a  finding  reached  by  comparing 
the  scores  of  each  group  with  the  scores  made  when  the  right  answer  was 
rewarded  every  time.  In  fact,  in  some  cases  age  and  intelligence  proved  to 
be  absolute  disadvantages;  college  professors,  for  example,  were  stumped 
by  some  of  the  problems  handled  by  the  average  6-year-olds.  In  a  test  like 

408 


this,  Dr.  Eisenberg  observes,  a  person  of  intelligence  and  sophistication 
keeps  making  hypotheses  and  discarding  them,  since  none  of  them 
rewards  him  consistently;  a  6-year-old  thinks  less  and  performs  better.  The 
investigator  points  out  that  even  animals  can  learn  certain  types  of 
responses  without  being  rewarded  every  time. 

These  results  again  are  taken  to  indicate  that  normal  children  use  two 
approaches  in  solving  problems  of  the  kind  presented  in  these  tests.  Young 
children  make  the  responses  that  a  trial-and-error  approach  leads  them  to 
think  will  pay  off  at  least  some  of  the  time.  As  children  grow  older,  they 
tend  to  look  for  some  hypothesis— some  rule— that  will  give  them  the 
answer  every  time.  Bright  children  show  the  same  tendency. 

Dr.  Osier  has  these  projects  under  way: 

1.  An  analysis  of  children's  information-processing  ability  as  shown  by 
their  responses  to  a  new  series  of  tests  in  which  the  amount  of  irrelevant 
information  is  systematically  varied.   The  investigator  is  learning  how 
much  information,  of  the  kinds  presented  in  this  research,  can  be  handled 
by  normal  children  of  a  given  age.  She  is  also  learning  just  where  the 
concept-forming  process  breaks  down  when  a  failure  occurs. 

2.  Comparisons  between  normal  and   retarded  children  and  between 
normal  and  disturbed  children. 

3.  Comparisons  between  the  normal  children  studied  so  far,  all  of  them 
from  good  neighborhoods,  and  children   from  slum  areas.  Since  other 
investigators  have  found  that  deprived  children  lag  in  intellectual  develop- 
ment, the  Hopkins  group  expects  that  the  slum-area  children  will  have 
considerably  more  difficulty  with  the  tests.  This  would  be  an  indication 
either  that  they  had  less  native  ability  to  form  concepts  or  draw  logical 
inferences,  or  that  for  some  reason  they  were  hampered  in  using  that 
ability.  The  research  group  believes  the  second  explanation  to  be  the  true 
one.   Specifically,  they  believe  that  difficulty  with  the  tests  could  be 
traced  to  backgrounds  so  impoverished  that  the  children  had  had  little 
experience  with  materials  like  those  used  in  the  tcsts-not  only  in  the 
variety  of  pictures  but  also  in  objects  of  different  sizes,  shapes,  and  colors. 
If  the  comparison  gives  the  expected  results,  the  investigators  will  test 
their  line  of  reasoning  by  training  some  of  the  slum-area  children  before- 
hand to  recognize  these  materials. 

Through  this  phase  of  the  work,  in  sum,  the  investigators  are  looking 
for  evidence  that  a  lag  in  a  child's  reasoning  ability  may  be  caused  by  a 
deficiency  in  experience  instead  of  in  intellect,  and  can  therefore  be 
prevented  or  corrected. 


Service  and  Research 

In  discussing  the  implications  of  recent  findings  by  his  group  and 
others,  Dr.  Eisenberg  stresses  two  compelling  mental  health  needs;  to  act 
more  effectively  on  the  basis  of  what  we  already  know  and  to  make 
greater  efforts  to  get  the  answers  to  what  we  still  do  not  know. 

409 


One  thing  we  already  know  is  that  distuibed  children  are  more  likely 
than  other  children  to  become  disturbed  adults;  hence  it  is  good 
preventive  psychiatiy  to  reach  these  childien.  And  child  psychiatrists 
could  reach  many  more  of  them,  the  investigator  observes,  by  following 
the  implications  of  two  of  the  findings  reported  earlier,  as  to  the  value  of 
brief  psychotherapy  for  neurotic  disorders  in  childhood  and  of  psy- 
chiatrically  supervised  treatment  for  delinquents  in  an  institution. 

A  clinical  study  by  Dr.  Eisenberg  a  few  years  ago  when  he  was  a 
consultant  to  Baltimore's  welfare  department  suggests  a  third  way  of 
making  our  psychiatiic  resources  go  father.  At  that  time  he  talked  with  a 
number  of  disturbed  children  from  foster  homes,  discussed  each  child 
with  the  social  worker  in  charge  of  the  case,  and  made  recommendations. 
These  had  to  do  with  such  matters  as  advising  the  foster  parents  to  modify 
their  treatment  of  the  child  in  some  respect,  finding  a  different  school,  or 
getting  the  child  interested  in  such  an  organization  as  the  Boy  Scouts. 
They  did  not  include  psychotherapy,  because  there  was  none  to  be  had,  or 
medication. 

When  the  records  of  the  welfare  department  were  checked  a  year  later, 
those  children  for  whom  the  recommendations  had  been  cairied  out  were 
found  to  have  made  a  much  better  adjustment  than  the  others.  (In  the 
other  cases  the  recommendations  had  not  been  carried  out  for  such 
leasons,  generally,  as  that  the  caseworkei  had  become  overloaded  or  had 
been  transferred,  or  that  a  certain  facility  was  not  available  in  the  child's 
neighborhood.)  This  study  was  not  so  rigorous  as  one  might  like,  Dr. 
Eisenberg  observes,  but  it  does  offer  evidence  that  if  psychiatrists  are  used 
to  enhance  the  effectiveness  of  other  mental  health  workers,  a  grcatci 
number  of  disturbed  children  can  be  helped. 

Dr.  Eisenberg  points  out  that  while  measures  to  reach  disturbed 
children  are  essential,  preventive  psychiatiy  will  be  most  effective  if  it 
helps  correct  the  conditions  that  lead  to  disturbances  in  children.  To  this 
end  he  holds  that  psychiatrists  must  go  beyond  the  clinic  and  the  consult- 
ing room  and  work  with  other  professional  people  to  broaden  the 
availability,  or  improve  the  effectiveness,  or  both,  of  family  planning  pro- 
grams, good  health  care,  decent  housing,  training  for  displaced  woikcrs, 
casework  services  to  minimize  family  breakdown,  substitute-care  for 
homeless  children,  enriched  school  programs,  and  the  like.  Some  deprived 
children  manage  to  grow  into  functioning  adults,  he  observes,  but  far  too 
many  contribute  to  statistics  on  delinquency  and  disease;  "They  become 
premature  and  inadequate  parents  themselves,  fated  to  repeat  for  a 
succeeding  generation  the  cycle  of  deprivation." 

In  spite  of  research  achievements  in  the  last  decade  or  so,  Dr.  Eisenberg 
emphasizes,  our  knowledge  about  pieventing  and  treating  psychiatric 
illness  has  serious  limitations.  For  example,  we  don't  know  how 
'"•IPS  work,  so  we  cannot  see  clearly  how  to  get  better  ones, 
"'-Jtely  acceptable  system  for  classifying  children's 
-an  impediment  both  in  treatment  and  in  research, 
sons-including  the  placebo  effect,  the  lack  of  fully 
ir  measuring  changes  in  attitudes  and  personalities, 


and  the  possibility  that  even  m  double-blind  studies  the  investigator  will 
detect  the  children  who  have  been  receiving  the  medication-it  is 
extremely  difficult  to  gauge  scientifically  the  worth  of  a  given  form  of 
treatment. 

Psychiatrists  are  charged  with  relieving  suffering,  Dr.  Eisenberg  notes, 
but  if  they  aie  to  do  this  most  effectively  psychiatry  must  acquire  ad- 
ditional knowledge  and  new  skills.  Hence,  though  there  is  an  urgent  need 
for  more  psychiatric  service,  theie  is  also  an  urgent  need  for  a  heavier 
investment  of  psychiatric  peisonncl  in  research. 

Reseat ch  Giant:  MH  2583 

Date  of  Interview     Apr.  14-15,  1965 

References: 

Conners,  C.  K.,  Eisenberg,  L.,  &  Sharpc,  L.  Effects  of  rnethylplicnidate  (Ritalin)  on 
paired-associate  learning  and  Porteus  Maze  performance  in  emotionally  disturbed 
children./.  Consult.  Psychol,  1964,  28,  1 

Cytryn,  L  >  Gilbert,  Anita,  &  Eisenberg,  L.  The  effectiveness  of  tranquilizmg  drugs 
phis  supportive  psychotherapy  in  treating  behavior  disorders  of  children:  a  double- 
blind  study  of  eighty  outpatients.  A  met:  L  Orthopsychiat.,  1960,  30,  1. 

Eisenberg,  L.  The  Strategic  Deployment  of  the  Child  Psychiatrist  in  Preventive  Psy- 
chiatry Presented  at  the  World  Congress  of  Psychiatry,  Montreal,  1961. 

Eisenberg,  L.  The  sins  of  the  fathers:  urban  decay  and  social  pathology.  Amer.  J. 
Orthopsychiat.,  1962,  32,  1 

Eisenberg,  L,  Role  of  drugs  in  treating  disturbed  children.  Children,  1964,  11,5. 

Eisenberg,  L.,  Conners,  C.  K.,  &  Sharpe,  L.  A  Controlled  Study  of  the  Differential 
Application  of  Outpatient  Psychiatric  Treatment  for  Children.  Presented  at  the 
Sixth  International  Congress  of  Pscy ho  therapy,  London,  1964. 

Eisenberg,  L  ,  Gilbert,  Anita,  Cytryn,  L.,  &  Moiling,  P.  A.  The  effectiveness  of  psy- 
chotherapy alone  and  in  conjunction  with  perphenazine  or  placebo  in  the  treatment 
of  neurotic  and  hyperkmetic  children.  Amer.  J.  Psychiat.,  1961,  1 17,  12. 

Eisenbeig,  L.t  Lachman,  R.,  Moiling,  P.  A,,  Lockner,  A.,  Mizelle,  J.  D,,  &  Conners,  C. 
K.  A  psychopharmacologic  experiment  in  a  training  school  for  delinquent  boys. 
Amei  J.Oithopsychfat,,  1963,33,3. 

Osier,  Sonia  F.,  &  Shapiro,  Sandra  L.  Studies  in  concept  attainment:  IV.  The  role  of 
paitial  reinforcement  as  a  function  of  age  and  intelligence.  Child  Develptn.,  1964, 
35. 


411 


Investigator1 

Marian  K.  DeMyer,  M.D. 

Indiana  University  School  of  Medicine 

Indianapolis,  Ind. 

Prepared  by. 

Herbert  Yahraes 

In  a  bioad-front  research  attack  on  early  childhood  schizophrenia,  the 
Indiana  University  School  of  Medicine  is  searching  for  causes,  collecting 
information  to  facilitate  diagnosis,  and  experimenting  with  promising  new 
ways  of  treatment. 

The  piogram  is  headed  by  Dr.  Marian  K.  DeMyer,  a  children's  psy- 
chiatrist and  director  of  the  medical  school's  Clinical  Research  Center  for 
Early  Childhood  Schizophrenia,  housed  in  the  La  Rue  D.  Carter  Memorial 
Hospital,  Indianapolis.  This  hospital  contains  the  State's  residential  treat- 
ment institution  foi  disturbed  children. 

The  children  most  intensively  studied  are  inpatients  at  the  Center. 
When  admitted  they  are  between  2  and  5  yars  old.  In  general,  they  cannot 
get  along  with  adults  or  other  children,  fly  easily  into  rage,  either  do  not 
speak  at  all  or  else  use  language  in  odd  and  incomprehensible  ways,  do  not 
listen  to  directions,  find  little  pleasure  in  play,  and  show  a  remarkably 
narrow  range  of  inteiests.  Some  of  them  twill  or  rock  themselves  for  long 
periods;  others  bite  or  hit  themselves.  They  often  seem  unsure  of  who  or 
what  they  are. 

Some  of  these  children  have  been  diagnosed  as  autistic,  a  type  of  child- 
hood schizophrenia  maiked  by  a  withdrawal  from  other  people;  others  us 
symbiotic,  a  type  in  which  the  child  physically  clings  to  his  mother. 
Because  some  of  the  children  display,  at  different  times,  each  of  these 
characteristics,  Dr.  DeMyei  classes  them  as  symbiotic-autistic.  Still  other 
patients  suffer  from  what  Dr.  DeMyer  labels  chronic  undifferentiated 
schizophrenia.  Their  behavior  is  less  abysmally  abnormal  than  that  of  the 
autistic  and  symbiotic  children;  they  have  some  conversational  ability  and 
usually  show  some  conformance  to  social  amenities. 

Investigations  under  way  are  directed  toward: 

•  presenting  detailed  descriptions  of  the  behavior  of  various  types  of 
schizophrenic  children  and  of  other  children  with  psychiatric  clis- 
ordeis; 

•  learning  how,  if  at  all,  the  preschool  schizophrenic  child  differs 
biologically  from  normal  children  and  whether  or  not  the  differences 
are  responsible  for  his  illness; 

412 


«   learning  whether  or  not  certain  influences  in  the  home,  notably  the 
personalities  of  the  parents  and  the  way  the  parents  treat  the  child, 
contribute  to  the  onset  of  childhood  schizophrenia, 
•   determining  the  value  of  several  new  approaches  to  treatment. 
So  far  the  Center's  research  piogram  has  led  to  two  results  of  major 
importance. 

First,  out  of  a  group  of  149  young  psychiatric  patients,  51  percent 
were  found  to  have  clearly  abnormal  electroencephalograms  and  15 
percent  had  experienced  at  least  one  epileptic-like  seizure. 
In  each  instance  the  proportion  would  have  been  highei  had  borderline 
cases  been  counted.  The  abnormalities  would  be  found  in  less  than  1 
percent  of  any  randomly  selected  noimal  population. 

The  patients  included  neurotics,  childhood  schizophienics  of  all  types, 
and  children  with  a  severe  but  nonpsychotic  behavior  clisordei,  marked  by 
such  activities  as  stealing,  setting  fires,  attacking  other  children  and  adults, 
and  refusing  to  get  along  in  school.  A  high  propoition  of  abnormal  EEC's 
was  found  among  all  the  patients  except  the  neurotics. 

The  results  are  important  because  they  indicate  that  many  children  who 
are  mentally  ill  have  a  physical  impaiiment  of  the  brain.  The  possibility 
that  the  impairment  signals  an  effect  of  the  illness  rather  than  a  cause,  Dr. 
DeMyer  observes,  cannot  yet  be  ruled  out.  Current  studies  of  schizo- 
phrenic and  nonschizophrenic  children  may  turn  up  clues  to  the  origin  of 
the  impairment. 

Befoie  the  EEC  study,  Dr.  DeMyer  had  leaned  to  the  psychogenic 
explanation  of  most  psychiatric  illness  and  had  given  only  superficial 
consideration  to  organic  factors.  "As  the  result  of  this  study,  done  in  a 
careful  way  and  using  the  severest  ciiteria  for  EEC  dysrhythmias,"  she 
notes,  "I  knew  that  our  research  program  would  have  to  include  a  careful 
study  of  the  central  nervous  system  and  other  biological  matters." 

Second,  conditioning  principles  similar  to  those  used  in  training 
animals-and  to  those  often  used,  unknowingly,  by  parents  in  train- 
ing their  children -have  been  successfully  applied  to  improving  the 
behavior  of  severely  schizophrenic  children  and  broadening  the 
activities  of  which  they  are  capable. 

Under  the  Center's  new  treatment  program,  called  the  semester  system, 
the  child  goes  through  cycles  of  5  months  at  the  center  and  7  months  at 
home,  and  the  staff  works  first  of  all  to  make  him  easier  to  live  with.  It 
tries  to  correct  those  behaviors  the  parents  have  found  most  disruptive  of 
family  life  and  it  instructs1  the  parents  how  to  handle  him.  In  accordance 
with  conditioning  theory,  desired  behavior  is  rewarded,  both  with  food 
and  with  a  "social  reinforcer"  such  as  praise,  and  undesiied  behavior  is 
ignored  or  is  associated  with  an  "aversion  stimulus"  such  as  physical 
restraint. 

Proposed  by  Dr.  Don  Churchill,  children's  psychiatrist  and  the  Center's 
assistant  director,  the  semester  system  was  introduced  in  the  fall  of  1965 
with  the  admission  of  four  new  patients.  In  all  these  cases,  after  the 
children  had  gone  home  for  a  7-month  stay,  the  parents  reported  they 
were  having  much  less,  if  any,  difficxilty  with  the  particular  kinds  of 

413 


behavior  they  had  identified  as  the  most  troublesome.  The  system  is  now 
being  used  with  all  the  Center's  inpatients-1 1  at  present. 

How  many  times  a  child  will  return  for  the  5-month  hospital  cycle 
lemains  uncertain.  But  Dr.  Churchill  observes  that  he  is  not  inteiestcd 
merely  in  impiovmg  behavior  and  has  not  thrown  overboard  the  psycho- 
analytic concepts  in  which  he  was  trained;  he  just  doesn't  find  these 
concepts  very  useful  in  working  with  extiemely  sick  children.  According 
to  psychoanalytic  theory,  the  therapist  establishes  a  relationship  with  a 
mentally  ill  person  and  uses  this  relationship  to  induce  normal  behavior, 
But  with  most  children  like  those  at  the  Center,  Dr.  Churchill  notes,  it  has 
been  extremely  difficult  both  to  establish  a  relationship  and  to  effect 
much  improvement.  If  a  certain  amount  of  normal  behavior  is  first  trained 
into  such  severely  ill  children,  the  investigator  suggests,  they  may  show 
more  interest  in  the  adults  about  them,  and  the  therapeutic  relationship 
will  follow  and  be  fruitful.  In  any  event,  the  trained-in  normal  activities 
may  crowd  out  at  least  some  of  the  customary  abnormal  ones. 

As  one  promising  way  of  training  in  normal  behavior,  the  Center  has 
been  experimenting  with  a  technique  for  inducing  a  schizophrenic 
youngster  to  imitate  specific  actions  of  the  therapist.  The  fiist  results  arc 
exciting:  two  of  the  most  severely  sick  children  have  been  trained  to 
imitate  several  hundred  simple  activities  and  to  say  a  few  dozen  words.  To 
Dr.  Joseph  N.  Hingtgen,  the  psychologist  in  charge  of  this  work,  the 
preliminary  outcome  suggests  that  the  schizophrenic  process  is  not 
completely  irreversible-that  perhaps  even  terribly  withdrawn  children  can 
be  brought  up  to  something  approaching  the  normal  level. 

Dr.  Hingtgen  emphasizes  that  he  is  speaking  on  the  basis  of  only  a  few 
cases-two  of  his  own,  and  several  reported  by  another  psychologist,  Dr. 
0.  Ivar  Lovaas,  who  works  at  the  University  of  California,  Los  Angeles, 
and  whose  ideas  have  influenced  the  Indianapolis  group.  Further,  iho 
technique  requires  such  an  immense  amount  of  work  that  only  a  lelatively 
few  children  could  be  helped  if  it  had  to  be  administered  professionally, 
But  Dr.  Hingtgen  finds  that  it  can  easily  be  taught  to  parents.  The  basic 
idea  is  that  normal  children,  to  a  great  extent,  learn  by  imitating  adults. 
Gravely  psychotic  children,  however,  pay  no  attention  to  adulls  and, 
therefore,  fail  to  imitate.  They  have  to  be  trained  to  imitate  so  that  they 
can  learn  to  behave  normally.  The  training  can  be  accomplished  through 
many  long  and  laborious  sessions  in  which  the  child  is  rewarded  for  doing 
what  the  therapist-or  the  parent-does,  beginning  with  such  simple 
activities  as  holding  up  a  finger.  Eventually  the  child  begins  to  imitate  the 
adult  spontaneously,  and  even  to  do  what  the  adult  requests. 

five  learning  technique  fits  in  nicely  with  the  ideas  and 

he  new  semester  system  and,  depending  on  the  results  of 

nay  eventually  be  tried  with  all  the  children  at  the  Center. 

ition  about  the  findings  noted,  together  with  a  brief  report 
»ther  research  of  the  Indianapolis  group,  follows. 


Abnormal  EEC's  in  Young  Psychiatric  Patients 

The  EEC  study  was  pait  of  the  research  seeking  to  describe  the 
anatomical,  biochemical,  and  physiological  makeup  of  the  preschool 
schizophrenic  child.  Among  the  children  tested  were  58  diagnosed  as 
autistic,  symbiotic,  or  symbiotic-autistic,  44  with  chronic  undifferentiated 
schizophrenia,  37  with  a  nonpsychotic  behavior  disorder,  10  as  neurotics, 
and  13  with  normal  controls.  They  were  from  4  to  10  years  old.  Since 
most  of  the  psychiatric  patients  fought  against  the  test,  the  EEC's  in 
virtually  all  the  cases  were  taken  after  the  children  had  been  calmed  by  a 
tianquilizer,  promazine— given  in  too  small  a  dose,  Dr.  DeMyer  reports,  to 
affect  the  pattern  of  electrical  discharges. 

As  noted  earlier,  51  percent  of  the  psychiatric  patients  were  found  to 
have  abnormal  EEC's,  a  proportion  that  would  have  been  slightly  higher 
had  the  neurotics,  only  one  of  whom  showed  an  abnormality,  been 
omitted.  All  of  the  normal  children  had  normal  EEC's.  Among  the 
children  with  nonpsychotic  behavior  disorders  of  the  acting-out  type  and 
among  those  with  the  various  types  of  schizophrenia,  there  were  no 
significant  differences  in  the  proportion  of  abnormalities;  also,  the  types 
of  abnormalities  were  the  same  and  were  present  to  about  the  same 
extent.  Most  chaiacteristic  was  the  type  of  activity  described  as 
paroxysmal  spike  and  wave  (PSW),  which  consists  of  a  mixture  of 
synchronous  spikes  and  slow  waves,  This  type  appeared  in  almost  two- 
thirds  of  the  children  with  abnormal  EEC's. 

The  EEC  study,  which  was  directed  by  two  neurologists— Drs.  Philip  T. 
White  and  William  DeMyer— in  cooperation  with  the  investigator,  opens 
several  important  questions,  including; 

1.  What  relationship  has  the  central  nervous  system  disorder,  demon- 
strated in  half  of  the  psychiatric  patients,  to  the  abnormal  behavior  of 
these  children? 

On  the  basis  of  this  and  previous  studies,  the  investigators  answer,  it 
seems  established  that  at  least  the  EEC  abnormality  labeled  PSW  has  more 
than  a  chance  relationship  to  disturbed  behavior.  This  means  that  in 
seeking  the  causes  of  childhood  schizophrenia  and  other  psychiatric 
disorders,  the  role  of  cerebral  dysfunction  cannot  be  ignored.  But  the 
exact  relationship  between  abnormality  and  behavior  remains  to  be 
discovered.  One  puzzling  fact  is  that  most  children  with  epilepsy,  who 
have  EEC  abnormalities  similar  to  those  found  in  this  study,  have  normal 
social  and  intellectual  skills  and  behave  normally  except  during  a  seizure, 
In  the  children  with  schizophrenia  and  behavior  disorders  the  EEC  ab- 
normalities presumably  could  be  the  result  of  biochemical  changes  caused 
by  the  stress  associated  with  psychiatric  illness.  But  they  could  also  be  one 
aspect  of  an  organic  disturbance,  congenital  or  acquired  very  early  in  life, 
directly  responsible  for  the  illness. 

2.  Why  did  only  about  half  of  the  psychiatric  patients  show  abnormal 
brain  waves? 

One  possible  answer,  says  the  investigator,  has  to  do  with  the  electro- 
encephalographic  technique  itself,  which  cannot  pick  up  all  abnormalities, 

415 


Another  possibility  has  to  do  with  the  way  abnormalities  are  defined.  The 
Indianapolis  study  counted  only  those  variations  in  the  EEC  that  weic 
distinctly  different  from  normal;  now  the  records  are  being  reexamined  to 
determine  the  effect  of  taking  borderline  changes  into  account.  Pievious 
EEC  studies  of  psych uitrically  disturbed  children.  Dr.  DeMyer  recalls, 
reported  abnormalities  in  as  low  as  2  percent  of  the  patients,  and  in  as 
high  as  90  percent.  She  thinks  that  biases  both  in  psychiatric  diagnosis  ami 
in  EEC  diagnosis  explain  part  of  this  wide  range. 

As  still  anothei  possibility,  the  sampling  may  have  been  inadequate, 
meaning  that  many  of  the  records  which  were  normal  for  the  particular 
hour  when  they  were  made  might  have  been  abnormal  if  made  at  some 
other  time.  The  investigator  points  out  that  even  peisons  with  epilepsy 
often  have  normal  records  except  when  seizures  are  occurring;  perhaps, 
she  reasons,  the  iccords  of  most  psychiatric  patients  would  show  ab- 
normalities if  taken  at  times  of  intense  anxiety  or  shortly  afterward.  A 
study  in  which  biain  waves  would  be  picked  up  and  telemeteied  to  a 
recording  station,  by  an  appaiatus  worn  by  the  child,  throughout  a  day  of 
usual  activity  is  being  planned. 

There  is  the  possibility,  too,  that  a  numbei  of  the  children  with  normal 
records  will  show  abnormal  ones  a  little  later  in  life. 

The  Center  is  preparing  a  laboratory  to  get  further  information  about 
the  brain's  electrical  activity  in  psychiatric  patients  by  recording  cerebral- 
evoked  potentials,  meaning  the  brain's  electrical  responses  to  external 
stimuli-a  light,  a  touch,  a  sound.  The  subjects  will  be  schizophrenic 
children  whose  EEC's  are  normal.  Normal  and  retarded  children  will  be 
tested  the  same  way. 

3.  Do  the  families  of  patients  who  show  no  EEC  abnormalities  differ 
significantly  from  the  families  of  those  who  do? 

In  the  EEC  study,  no  attempt  was  made  to  evaluate  the  families. 
However,  a  study  nearing  completion,  in  which  families  with  a  schizo- 
phrenic child  are  being  compared  with  one  another  and  with  matched 
families  whose  children  are  normal,  should  provide  an  answer.  The  subject 
bears  upon  the  genesis  of  childhood  schizophrenia.  Another  investigator, 
working  with  somewhat  older  children,  found  that  children  showing  no 
organic  impairment  came  from  more  deeply  disturbed  families  than  the 
others,  the  implication  being  that  in  some  cases  an  organic  impairment 
contributes  to  the  onset  of  schizophrenia,  but  in  other  cascs-whcre  the 
family  is  seriously  disturbed-schizophrenia  can  develop  whether  or  not 
something  is  organically  wrong. 

Dr.  DeMyer  and  her  associates  have  also  used  the  EEG  to  study  the 
sleep  and  dream  patterns  of  seven  schizophrenic  children  hospitalized  at 
the  Center.  Their  sleep  habits  at  home,  the  parents  reported,  had  been 
extremely  erratic.  At  the  Center,  though,  the  staff  considered  their  sleep 
to  be  remarkably  regular,  and  the  records  of  brain  waves  and  eye  move- 
ments during  sleep  showed  that  these  children  spent  the  same  proportion 
of  time  in  the  dreaming  stage  as  normal  children.  This  study,  then,  finds 
no  evidence  to  back  up  the  suggestion  that  patients  may  hallucinate  in  the 

416 


waking  state  because  they  have  been  unable  to  hallucinate  as  much  as 
other  persons  during  sleep. 


Earlier  Experiments  in  Conditioning 

The  Center's  use  of  conditioning  techniques-with  piomising  results- 
follows  a  series  of  experiments  to  find  ways  of  getting  very  sick 
youngsters  to  come  out  of  their  shells  and  act  moie  noimally  toward 
people  and  things. 

In  one  of  the  earlier  experiments,  directed  by  Dr.  C.  B.  Ferster,  an 
autistic  child  would  spend  an  hour  or  two,  every  day,  in  a  room  contain- 
ing such  fascinating  equipment  as  a  pinball  machine,  an  electric  train  set,  a 
phonograph,  a  picture  viewer,  a  telephone  set,  a  trained  monkey  in  a  cage, 
and  vending  machines.  Each  device  could  be  operated  by  the  child  if  he 
put  a  coin  in  the  proper  slot  (in  the  case  of  the  monkey,  the  coin  tinned 
on  a  light  in  the  cage  and  the  animal  went  into  his  act);  and  the  child 
could  get  the  coins  by  pressing  a  key  that  operated  a  coin-dispensing 
machine. 

Rats  and  cats  will  learn  to  keep  pressing  a  bar,  and  pigeons  to  keep 
pecking  at  a  disc,  if  such  activity  occasionally  brings  them  food.  This 
learning  process  is  known  as  operant  conditioning  because  it  gets  the 
subject  to  work,  or  operate,  in  a  desired  manner.  The  same  principle  held 
good  with  the  schizophrenic  children.  They  learned  that  by  pressing  the 
key  often  enough,  they  would  get  a  coin  good  for  any  of  the  devices,  and 
soon  they  spent  most  of  their  time  working  for  coins  and  spending  them. 
The  most  popular  devices  by  far  were  the  candy  vending  machines. 

Dr.  Ferster's  next  question  was  whether  or  not  the  same  technique 
could  be  used  to  help  the  child  understand  more  difficult  situations  and 
engage  in  more  complex  bchavior-an  important  question  because  one  of 
the  most  marked  characteristics  of  schizophrenic  children  is  their 
extremely  narrow  range  of  activity. 

In  one  of  the  new  situations,  coins  were  delivered  only  when  a  panel  on 
the  dispensing  machine  was  lighted.  In  another,  a  device  could  be  operated 
only  if  a  coin  was  placed  in  a  slot  when  the  slot  was  lighted.  A  more 
difficult  task  required  the  child,  if  he  were  to  get  a  coin,  to  compare  two 
colors  or  figures  or  pictures  and  touch  the  one  that  matched  a  third, 
which  he  had  been  previously  trained  to  touch.  Dr.  Ferster's  question  was 
answered  in  the  affirmative,  even  the  autistic  children  came  to  understand 
and  master  the  new  situations,  though  more  slowly  than  normal  children. 

To  sum  up,  the  study  found  that  children  who  rarely  had  taken  any 
account  of  their  environment  could  soon  begin,  through  the  use  of 
conditioning  techniques,  not  only  to  notice  the  environment  but  also— by 
pressing  keys,  matching  pictures,  and  dropping  coins— to  manipulate  it. 
True,  the  environment  was  artificial  and  the  behavior  needed  to  control  it 
a  little  out  of  the  ordinary.  But  the  results  were  taken  to  indicate,  in  these 
schizophrenic  children,  "at  least  the  existence  of  normal  processes  at  a 

417 


very  basic  level,"  with  no  suggestion  of  an  underlying  deficit  except  in  tl 
rate  at  which  the  children  learned. 

The  next  question  was  whether  01  not  a  similar  laboratoiy  situatio 
could  be  used  to  teach  socially  adaptive  behavior~specifically,interactio 
with  other  children.  Normal  youngsters  in  a  preschool  group  are  almo: 
constantly  interacting:  by  playing  together,  foi  example,  grabbing  eac 
other's  toys,  and  fighting.  But  schizophrenic  children  hold  thcmselvt 
aloof. 

In  research  directed  by  Dr.  Hingtgen,  the  Center  tried  to  devclo 
cooperative  behavior  in  some  of  its  children.  Only  two  machines  wcr 
used—  the  coin  dispenser,  set  to  deliver  one  coin  for  every  1  5  presses  of  th 
key,  and  a  vending  machine  offering  candy,  crackers,  and  cereal.  Si 
children  participated.  After  each  had  been  trained  to  opeiate  th 
machines,  they  were  paired,  and  for  the  rest  of  the  experiment  bo  I 
members  of  a  pair  went  to  the  laboratory  room  together. 

For  a  while,  each  member  of  a  pair,  A  and  B,  was  free  to  operate  th 
coin  lever  at  any  time.  When  it  was  operated  by  A,  a  panel  behind  th 
lever  showed  red;  when  it  was  operated  by  B  the  panel  showed  green. 

Step  2  required  a  little  cooperation  because  A  could  operate  the  leve 
only  when  the  panel  showed  red,  and  B  only  when  it  showed  green.  Aftc 
one  child  received  a  coin,  the  other  child's  light  was  presented.  Tin 
children  learned  by  trial  and  error  that  they  had  to  take  turns. 

In  step  3,  the  children  had  to  use  a  two-key  panel  in  another  part  of  thi 
room.  One  key  could  be  illuminated  by  a  red  light;  the  other  by  a  green 
When  the  red  light  was  on  and  A  pressed  the  corresponding  key,  the  con 
lever  showed  red;  then  A  could  go  to  work  to  get  a  coin. 

In  the  final  step,  when  A  pressed  the  red-light  key,  the  coin  Icvei 
showed  green,  and  B  could  operate  the  machine;  when  B  pressed  the 
green-light  key,  the  light  on  the  coin  machine  went  red  for  4.  Thus  A  and 
B  had  to  work  for  each  other  by  providing  the  appropriate  coin-lever  light, 

Starting  with  step  2,  the  children  learned  to  work  for  each  other  in  an 
average  of  23  sessions. 

To  the  investigators'  surprise,  in  view  of  earlier  observations  of  schizo- 
phrenic children  at  play,  the  youngsters  in  this  particular  situation 
frequently  made  physical  contact  with  each  other.  One  would  lead  the 
other  to  the  correct  lever,  or  one  would  operate  the  correct  lever  by 
manipulating  the  other  youngster's  hand,  or  one  would  pull  the  other 
away  from  the  coin  lever  or  from  the  vending  machine. 

In  one  session  a  4-year-old  boy  was  working  at  a  very  low  rate.  He  had 
to  press  the  coin  lever  15  times  in  order  to  be  rewarded,  but  he  often 
stopped  and  began  twirling.  Since  his  partner,  a  6-year-old  girl,  could  not 
go  to  work  for  her  com  until  he  received  his,  she  began  pulling  him  back  to 
the  lever  whenever  he  wandered  off.  Once  she  slapped  him,  and  ills 

XTtoT.T  up',  AnotheVime'  a  slap  set  hta  *>  «y"i  "nd  * 

ht    hnln  I       ™<*me  "*  a11  After  a  long  period'  she  wen'  over  to 
him,  hugged  him  around  the  neck  and  led  him  back  to  the  lever-  his 

went  up<  In  later  sessions>  she  slapped  him  a 


418 


All  the  children  appeared  to  be  communicating  with  their  partners 
through  vocal  and  facial  expressions,  and  two  of  the  less  seriously 
disturbed  used  words-"That's  the  red  light."  "That's  enough,"  "Get  the 
coin." 

This  social  interaction,  however,  did  not  carry  over  from  the  ex- 
perimental room  to  life  outside.  Evidently  young  schizophrenic  children 
were  capable  of  modifying  their  usual  behavior,  but  how  could  the 
modifications  be  made  to  stick? 

The  next  experiment  tested  the  effect  of  directly  rewarding  the 
children  for  making  physical  contact  with  each  other.  The  mechanical 
devices  were  not  used:  an  observer  simply  handed  the  children  a  small 
piece  of  candy  or  a  bit  of  a  cookie  or  cracker  whenever  they  performed 
the  desired  behavior.  In  the  first  stage  the  childien  were  rewarded  when- 
ever one  touched  the  other.  The  required  behavior  was  made  gradually 
more  complex  until,  in  the  end,  the  reward  was  given  only  when  the  child 
touched  the  other  with  both  hands  and  said  something,  whether  com- 
prehensible or  not. 

This  time  there  was  a  little  more  carryover.  Parents  and  ward  personnel 
reported  that  the  children  would  come  up  to  them  once  in  a  while,  touch 
them,  and  make  a  sound  or  two.  But  this  social  behavior  died  out  in  a  few 
weeks,  presumably  because  it  no  longer  brought  the  kind  of  reward  that 
had  been  used  in  developing  it. 

The  research  group  then  tackled  one  phase  of  an  old  and  basic  problem: 
why  the  severely  schizophrenic  child  does  not  respond  like  a  normal  child 
to  what  he  hears  and  sees.  Clinicians  are  satisfied  that  he  can  hear  and  see, 
in  spite  of  the  many  indications  he  may  give  to  the  contrary,  particularly 
in  the  matter  of  hearing.  But  sometimes,  apparently,  he  shuts  out  his 
perceptions,  and  other  times,  apparently,  he  fails  to  organize  them— at 
least  he  does  not  act  upon  them  in  normal  fashion.  Part  of  the  trouble, 
some  investigators  have  thought,  may  be  an  inability  to  discriminate 
among  stimuli. 

Dr.  Hingtgen  used  an  auditory  discrimination  test  with  six  of  the 
children.  First  a  child  would  learn  that  by  pressing  a  lever  often  enough  he 
would  get  a  coin,  which  he  could  use  to  obtain  candy  from  a  vending 
machine.  Then  the  experimenter  fixed  the  dispenser  so  that  it  would  drop 
coins  only  when  music  was  playing.  Like  rats,  pigeons,  and  normal 
children  in  earlier  experiments,  the  schizophrenics  soon  learned  to  work  at 
the  dispenser  only  during  periods  of  music;  in  other  words,  they  learned 
to  discriminate  between  music  and  silence. 

The  children  also  showed  they  could  discriminate  between  speech  (the 
voice  of  someone  reading  a  story)  and  silence,  and  between  speech  and 
music.  Then  the  investigators  tried  single  words,  in  pairs.  When  a  voice 
said  chalk,  for  example,  the  machine  was  operating;  when  the  voice  said 
ball,  it  wasn't.  Of  the  six  children  being  tested,  all  except  one  learned  to 
distinguish  even  between  wordSj  like  mama  and  apple,  that  are  rather 
similar  phonetically.  Again,  though,  the  learning  took  longer  than  with 
normal  children. 

419 


Learning  by  Imitation 

If  schizophrenic  children  have  this  power  of  auditory  discrimination, 
why  don't  they  use  it  all  the  time?  Maybe,  Dr.  Hingtgen  ventures,  it's  a 
matter  of  paying  attention,  and  perhaps  they  pay  attention  only  when 
they  are  going  to  get  something-and  when  that  something  meets  such  a 
basic  need  as  for  Food. 

The  researchers  were  discussing  ways  of  testing  this  and  other 
possibilities,  and  of  putting  the  findings  to  practical  use,  when  Dr.  Lovaas, 
the  University  of  California  psychologist  mentioned  earlier,  came  to  the 
school  of  medicine  and  described  some  of  his  work  with  young  schizo- 
phrenics. He,  too,  was  using  reward s-and  punishments,  also-but  in  a 
nonautomated  situation,  and  with  just  one  adult  and  a  child  working 
together,  intensively.  He  theorized  that  a  schizophrenic  child  fails  to  learn 
because  he  fails  to  imitate,  and  he  fails  to  imitate  because  he  doesn't 
associate  paying  attention  with  reward.  Once  the  child  has  been  taught  to 
pay  attention  and  to  imitate,  he  can  start  learning  on  his  own. 

What  was  new  here  was  not  the  general  learning-by-imitation  concept 
but  its  systematic  and  intensive  application  to  schizophrenic  cliildien.  One 
of  the  aspects  that  impressed  the  Indianapolis  group  was  the  comparative 
simplicity  of  the  technique.  You  present  the  child  with  a  model,  or 
example,  of  the  type  of  tiling  you  want  him  to  do.  He  imitates  you.  You 
reward  his  imitation.  This  is  easier  than  waiting  for  the  child  to  perform, 
more  or  less  accidentally,  the  behavior  you  have  in  mind,  then  rewarding 
him  for  it  and  going  on  to  shape  that  behavior  by  rewarding  him  for 
increasingly  complex  variations.  Getting  two  children  to  the  point  where 
they  weie  touching  one  another  with  both  hands  and  saying  something 
required  as  many  as  30  daily  sessions  of  at  least  an  hour  each. 

But  the  most  hopeful  aspect  of  the  learning-by-imitation  concept  was 
the  possibility  that  eventually  the  schizophrenic  child  would  conic  to 
imitate  spontaneously.  As  Dr.  Churchill  puts  it:  "To  teach  a  child 
separately  every  little  thing  he  has  to  learn  in  the  course  of  his  growing  up 
is  patently  impossible.  But  if  we  can  teach  a  child  to  imitate  others,  there 
is  the  hope  that  the  learning  process  will  become  more  automatic-will  not 
stop  at  the  end  of  the  learning  session,  but  continue  through  the  day." 

The  Indianapolis  investigators  decided  to  test  the  new  technique- 
modified,  though,  to  omit  punishment- with  some  of  the  children  at  the 
Center. 

Tommy  was  first.  He  was  six-and-a-half,  a  mute  child  with  a  very 
narrow  range  of  behavior.  He  had  been  subjected  to  every  available  type 
of  therapy,  but  in  3  years  at  the  Center  had  shown  no  significant  improve- 
ment. 

He  was  taken  off  the  ward,  given  his  own  room,  and  kept  there  for  20 
days,  Every  morning  about  8  o'clock  Dr.  Hingtgen  and  Mrs.  Susan 
Coulter,  his  research  assistant,  would  come  in  when  Tommy  got  up,  and 
every  evening  about  8  o'clock  they  would  put  him  to  bed.  They  were 
the*e  at  least  eight  of  the  intervening  hours  and-so  that  he  would  come  to 

420 


associate  food  and  drink  with  the  adults  who  were  asking  him  to  imitate 
them— gave  him  all  his  meals. 

The  investigators  aimed  to  get  three  types  of  imitation.  One  was  imita- 
tion in  use  of  body  parts-touching  two  fingers  together,  for  example. 
Another  was  imitation  in  use  of  objects-from  so  simple  an  activity  as 
picking  up  a  pencil  to  so  complicated  a  one  as  using  scissors  to  cut  out  a 
picture.  The  third,  since  Tommy  was  mute,  was  imitation  of  sounds  and 
speech. 

Like  other  seriously  schizophrenic  children,  Tommy  generally  avoided 
looking  at  adults,  so  the  first  sessions  were  rather  wearing.  The  procedure 
went  like  this.  One  of  the  teachers— Dr.  Hingtgen  and  Mrs.  Coulter 
alternated-would  seat  Tommy  in  a  chair  directly  in  front  of  him  and  say, 
"Tommy,  look  here.  Clap  your  hands."  And  the  teacher  would  clap  Ms 
own  hands.  At  the  beginning,  Tommy  would  not  clap  his  hands.  So  the 
teacher  would  clap  his  hands  for  him,  to  demonstrate  what  was  wanted, 
and  give  him  a  piece  of  candy  or  sugar-coated  cereal  or  some  other  food. 
The  idea  was  that  Tommy  could  not  get  out  of  his  chair— he  was  held  in 
by  the  teacher's  legs  if  necessary-until  he  had  done  what  his  teacher 
wanted  him  to  do.  Fussing  and  crying  did  no  good.  After  20  minutes  01 
so,  Tommy  clapped  his  hands. 

When  the  boy  had  learned  15  or  20  imitative  responses  through  such  a 
grueling  process,  he  began  imitating  immediately.  For  example,  Dr. 
Hingtgen  folded  his  hands  in  a  rather  complex  fashion,  and  Tommy, 
though  he  had  never  before  seen  him  do  this,  did  it  himself  right  away. 

Sometimes  the  boy  would  try  doing  the  same  thing  over  and  over,  only 
to  find  that  repetitive  responses  were  not  rewarded;  so  he  would  stop 
making  them.  For  the  sake  of  the  reward,  he  learned  to  watch  his  teachers 
very  carefully, 

Tommy's  use  of  objects  had  been  limited  and  bizarre.  He  would  busy 
himself  for  hours  with  ritualistic-like  activities,  such  as  moving  a  piece  of 
string  back  and  forth  in  front  of  his  eyes  or  tearing  paper  to  bits.  Con- 
sequently, getting  him  to  imitate  the  investigators'  use  of  objects  was 
especially  difficult.  He  got  around  to  picking  up  a  pencil  and  drawing  a 
straight  line  on  a  piece  of  paper  only  after  3  hours  of  work-spread  over 
several  days,  for  the  sake  of  everyone's  nervous  system.  But  once  he  drew 
the  straight  line,  it  was  easier  to  get  him  to  draw  X's,  squares,  and  circles. 

Learning  to  fold  a  piece  of  paper  once  and  then  to  fold  it  again  took  5 
hours. 

Getting  him  to  use  scissors  was  a  traumatic  experience,  too.  He  watched 
Mrs.  Coulter  use  them  and  he  wanted  to  hold  them  but  had  no  idea  how. 
Eventually  she  used  tape  to  help  keep  them  in  place,  and  in  2  hours  she 
got  him  to  make  one  little  cut  in  paper.  After  a  number  of  additional 
sessions,  he  was  cutting  out  circles  and  squares. 

Working  like  that  7  days  a  week,  the  investigators  would  go  home  "a  bit 
edgy,"  Dr.  Hingtgen  recalls,  but  once  the  behavior  started  coming,  a 
session  with  Tommy  was  rewarding  as  well  as  exhausting. 

Getting  this  mute  youngster  to  make  sounds  and  say  words  proved  to 
be,  as  expected,  the  most  difficult  part  of  the  process.  Since  Tommy'had 

421 


no  idea  what  to  do  with  his  mouth  in  order  to  voice  a  word,  the  teachers 
began  by  having  him  imitate  the  position  of  the  mouth  necessary  foi 
forming  a  given  sound.  In  the  case  of  the  "ah"  sound,  for  example, 
Tommy  was  rewarded  at  first  merely  for  pointing  to  his  chin  and  opening 
his  mouth;  later  he  had  to  make  the  sound. 

To  teach  him  the  "P"  sound,  the  teachers  brought  in  a  harmonica, 
which  Tommy  knew  how  to  use.  As  he  blew  it,  they  would  take  it  away 
and  reward  him  for  continuing  to  blow.  After  a  long  session,  he  finally 
came  to  voice  the  required  sound  every  time  the  investigators  offered  it. 

At  the  end  of  the  3  weeks,  Tommy  would  say  18  words,  imitativcly. 
Some  woids  weie  very  clew— baby,  mama,  daddy,  puppy.  Some  wore 
appioximations— feer  instead  of  finger.  He  also  had  more  than  150 
imitative  responses,  including  standing,  running,  and  jumping,  thai 
required  him  to  make  use  of  his  body.  And  he  had  more  than  100  uses  of 
objects. 

He  was  doing  all  these  things  only  imitatively.  But  he  was  imitating 
consistently  and  paying  consistent  attention  to  the  two  adults  closest  to 
him.  At  this  point,  Tommy  was  returned  to  the  ward,  and  Dr.  Hmglgcn 
and  Mrs.  Coulter  began  teaching  the  learning-by-imitation  technique  to 
the  nurses  and  other  peisonnel  and  to  Tommy's  parents.  The  emphasis 
now  was  not  merely  on  increasing  his  imitative  repertory,  but  also  on 
getting  him  to  attach  meaning  to  his  vaiious  responses,  particularly  (he 
vocal  ones.  After  2  more  months  of  morning  and  afternoon  sessions  in 
the  hospital  5  days  a  week,  at  home  on  weekends-Tommy  had  about  30 
words  to  which  he  attached  meaning.  For  example,  when  a  teacher 
pointed  to  her  nose  Tommy  said  nose\  when  she  held  up  a  picture  ol*£i 
puppy,  he  said  puppy, 

Whatever  the  original  cause  of  early  childhood  schizophrenia,  Dr. 
Hingtgen  believes  a  youngster  persists  in  his  withdrawn  attitude  and 
bizarre  behavior  because  he  lias  learned  that  he  can  get  by  without  doing 
anything  his  parents  want.  "At  home,"  the  investigator  points  out,  "the 
parents  have  never  been  able  to  command  as  much  from  the  child  as  we 
have  because  they  have  not  been  able  to  invest  so  much  time.  Also,  they 
never  think  of  spending  3  hours  trying  to  get  him  to  hold  a  pair  of 
scissors,  for  example,  because  no  normal  child  requires  that  long. 

"Some  authorities  have  thought  that  these  children  really  cannot  learn, 
but  our  idea  is  that  they  are  very  adept  at  learning  how  not  to  learn  -how 
not  to  have  to  do  something."  At  the  beginning  of  Tommy's  training,  for 
example,  the  boy  would  giggle  or  laugh  instead  of  trying  to  do  what  had 
been  requested.  Then  he'd  act  as  if  he  were  going  to  cry.  Then  he'd  try  to 
fight  his  way  out  of  the  chair,  and  he  might  break  into  real  tears.  But  the 
investigators  would  not  go  away,  and  they  would  not  cease  making  their 
demand,  so  eventually  Tommy  would  come  out  with  the  behavior  they 
wanted.  Since  Tommy's  avoidance  behavior,  as  psychologists  call  it,  was 
never  successful,  within  3  weeks  he  stopped  using  it. 

The  same  technique  has  brought  similar  results  in  another  autistic  child, 

5-year-old  Peggy,  who  had  been  even  more  withdrawn  at  the  beginning 
than  Tnrnrnw 


Don't  the  children  get  tired  of  the  rewards— become  literally  fed  up? 
Tommy  and  Peggy  did  not,  although  they  weie  eating  almost  con- 
tinuously dm  ing  the  3  weeks  of  intensive  treatment,  and  each  gained 
about  a  pound  a  week.  But  the  rewards  were  very  small;  one  potato  chip 
would  be  broken  into  six  or  eight  pieces. 

Then,  too,  the  food  reward  was  coupled  with  two  other  reinforcement 
measmes.  One  was  the  easing  of  physical  lestraint  if  such  restraint  had 
been  necessary,  as  at  first  it  generally  had  been.  The  other  was  social 
reinforcement  Whenever  Tommy  made  the  appropriate  response,  the 
investigator  would  say,  "Good  boy,  Tommy  "-or  hug  him,  or  give  him  a 
ride  around  the  room,  or  just  touch  him.  In  an  earlier  study,  the 
Indianapolis  team  had  tiied  using  social  reinforcers  alone  to  improve  the 
behavior  of  children  like  Tommy,  and  the  gains  had  been  neither  very 
large  nor  consistent.  But  when  Tommy  and  Peggy  had  finished  their 
intensive  treatment,  the  investigators  found  it  possible  to  reduce  the 
frequency  of  the  food  rewards  and  count  more  and  mote  on  the  social 
reinforcers  alone.  "One  theory  of  learning,"  Dr.  Hingtgen  observes,  "is 
that  all  social  reinforcers  gain  their  strength  by  having  been  initially  paired 
with  food  and  warmth.  Hopefully,  at  some  point  in  the  training  of  these 
children,  social  reinforcers— paired  only  occasionally  with  food— will  be 
sufficient." 

Because  only  certain  behaviors  are  rewardcd-thoae  the  adults  request 
and  demonstrate— the  children  do  not  become  automatons,  doing  every- 
thing they  see  their  teachers  do.  Eventually,  it  is  hoped,  the  habits  they 
are  learning,  of  paying  attention  and  of  doing  what  is  asked  and  demon- 
strated, will  take  hold  to  such  an  extent  that  the  children  will  sponta- 
neously imitate  behavior-including  speech,  play  activities,  use  of  house- 
hold equipment—that  normal  children  imitate.  Tommy  and  Peggy  do  a 
little  of  this  now.  They  also  obey  some  simple  instructions  even  when  no 
demonstration  is  given. 

Dr.  Hingtgen  throws  in  a  cautioner.  Both  Tommy  and  Peggy  have  been 
significantly  improved  but  are  still  a  long  way  from  where  they  should  be, 
considering  their  ages.  So  the  question  is:  How  far  can  you  take  such 
children  through  the  use  of  this  technique?  On  the  basis  of  Dr.  Lovaas's 
work,  Dr.  Hingtgen  notes,  there  is  evidence  that  at  least  some  severely 
schizophrenic  children  can  be  brought  into  a  classroom  situation.  Whether 
or  not  they  will  ever  be  normal,  no  one  cay  say. 

Further,  this  investigator  emphasizes,  the  technique  has  been  used  with 
probably  not  moie  than  half  a  dozen  children.  "So  we  don't  know  yet 
whether  this  is  a  valid  technique  for  all  early  child  schizophrenics,"  he 
observes,  "or  whether  those  six  just  happened  to  be  ones  that  it  worked 
with." 

The  Indianapolis  group,  though,  has  made  headway  in  answering  some 
other  questions  about  the  technique.  For  one  thing,  its  application  can  be 
made  less  strenuous  for  the  teacher  than  Tommy's  case  may  suggest. 
Tommy  was  isolated  for  3  weeks  and  all  the  shaping  of  his  behavior  was 
done  by  two  persons.  Peggy,  too,  was  isolated,  but  half  a  dozen  persons 
guided  the  conditioning  process.  Dr.  Hingtgen  now  suspects,  and  will  try 

423 


to  confirm,  that  the  same  results  can  be  obtained  if  the  child  continues  to 
live  on  the  ward  instead  of  being  isolated  when  the  conditioning  process 
begins.  The  investigator  also  plans  to  test  his  idea  that  the  technique,  after 
the  intensive  preliminary  treatment,  can  be  used  with  two  or  three 
children  at  a  time.  As  he  sees  it,  hospital  care  might  then  consist  mainly 
of  a  series  of  classes  in  which  the  children  would  learn  to  imitate  various 
kinds  of  responses.  Sometimes  the  child  would  be  with  a  small  group; 
sometiines  alone  with  a  therapist  or  another  adult. 

In  many  cases,  this  investigator  believes,  it  will  be  good  for  the  parents 
to  come  light  into  the  hospital  and  work  with  the  child  on  a  regular  basis* 
as  Tommy's  and  Peggy's  mothers  have  been  doing.  "This  is  a  very  simple 
technique,"  he  points  out,  "and  can  be  taught  to  anybody:  simply 
rewarding  the  child  at  the  appropriate  time  and  withholding  reward  at  the 
appropriate  time." 

Among  the  basic  questions  still  to  be  answered  is  how  many  models  of 
behavior  a  schizophrenic  child  must  be  trained  to  imitate  before  he  begins 
imitating  on  his  own. 


The  Semester  Program 

As  noted  earlier,  the  Center  now  uses  conditioning  techniques  with  all 
its  patients  in  an  effort  to  correct  those  behaviors  in  a  child  that  have 
most  upset  the  family.  Though  the  application  is  less  intensive  than  the 
process  used  with  Tommy  and  Peggy,  the  techniques  are  basically  the 
same.  While  the  children  are  in  the  hospital,  the  parents,  individually  and 
in  groups,  are  instructed  how  to  take  care  of  them  when  they  come  home, 
as  they  do  for  7  months  of  the  year. 

"The  predominate  experience  everywhere  in  trying  to  help  a  young, 
severely  ill  child,"  Dr.  Churchill  remarks  in  explaining  these  rather 
revolutionary  innovations,  "has  been  of  working  hard  and  for  a  long  time 
and  then,  more  often  than  not,  of  seeing  him  transferred  to  a  State  insti- 
tution for  the  mentally  ill  or  the  retarded." 

After  a  child  has  been  a  patient  at  the  Center  or  some  comparable  place 
for  2  or  3  years,  the  psychiatrist  continues,  the  parents  "have  sort  of 
fallen  out  of  love  with  him."  It  is  then  extremely  difficult  to  get  him  back 
into  the  family  again,  and  this  is  partly  because  not  very  much  has  been 
done  with  either  the  child  or  the  parents  to  enable  him  to  live  in  the 
home. 

"In  nther  words,"  Dr.  Churchill  observes,  "the  emphasis  has  been  on 
an  all-nr-nni-hing  proposition— on  curing."  A  psycho- 
Mmself,  he  goes  on,  "has  been  trained 
•'tient  and  to  consider,  in  line  with 
aing  on  inside  the  patient,  and  why, 
Dssible  to  establish  this  relationship, 
lis  may  take  a  couple  of  years— and 
ent.  But  generally  the  child  still  is 


At  the  Centei,  where  the  experience  has  been  typical,  he  believes,  the 
instances  in  which  these  young  children  have  improved  sufficiently  to 
return  home  and  to  enter  a  public  school  have  been  so  few  and  far 
between  that  "we'ie  not  sure  whether  they  improved  to  this  extent 
because  of  what  we  did  or  in  spite  of  what  we  did." 

Now,  under  the  new  semester  system,  the  Center  has  a  dual  objective: 
to  cure  the  children,  "if  they  can  be  cured/'  and  meanwhile  to  make  them 
easier  to  live  with  and  to  help  the  families  get  along  with  them. 

"Family  morbidity  in  these  cases  is  very  high,"  Dr.  Churchill  notes. 
"These  are  families  who  haven't  gone  to  a  restauiant  or  even  out  on  a 
family  picnic  for  several  years  because  they  cannot  manage  the  sick  child. 
The  parents  may  never  go  out  together  because  babysitters  won't  come  in. 
Often  the  house  is  stripped,  with  everything  of  value  kept  behind  locked 
doors,  and  the  family  is  living  within  bare  walls." 

In  working  with  the  parents,  the  therapists  focus  not  so  much  on  the 
emotional  conflicts  that  may  have  led  or  contributed  to  the  child's  illness 
but  on  specific  methods  of  dealing  with  his  most  disruptive  types  of 
behaviors -methods  used  by  the  Center  itself  while  the  child  is  an 
inpatient.  For  example,  if  the  child  is  sitting  at  the  table  and  throwing 
food,  the  parents  are  instiucted  to  give  him  a  warning,  If  he  throws  food 
again,  that's  the  end  of  the  meal  for  him  and  he  doesn't  eat  until  the  next 
meal, 

This,  too,  is  operant  conditioning.  It  pairs  an  undesirable  behavior, 
throwing  food,  with  a  unpleasant  result,  removal  of  food.  In  the  case  of  a 
child  who  begins  to  wreck  a  room  or  a  grocery  store,  the  unpleasant  result 
may  be  simply  physical  restraint. 

Many  times,  though,  the  best  way  to  handle  unwanted  behavior  is 
simply  to  ignore  it.  It  was  failure  to  do  this  when  the  behavior  first 
appeared,  Dr.  Churchill  believes,  that  may  have  helped  perpetuate  it.  In 
other  words,  even  if  disturbed  behavior  has  a  physiological  basis,  the  first 
adult  response  to  it  may  have  had  a  rewarding  and  thus  a  strengthening 
effect. 

Sometimes,  notably  in  the  case  of  temper  tantrums,  there  is  more  than 
one  way  of  ignoring  a  behavior,  and  choosing  the  effective  way  requires 
the  parent  to  consider  why  the  child  is  acting  as  he  is.  For  instance,  if  the 
child  has  been  using  tantrums  as  a  means  of  compelling  an  adult  to  come 
and  do  something  for  him  that  he  should  be  doing  for  himself,  the 
effective  form  of  ignoring  the  tantrum  is  to  stay  away.  But  if  the  child  has 
been  using  tantrums  to  hold  off  an  adult  and  prevent  necessary  ministra- 
tions, the  effective  form  of  ignoring  the  tantrum  is  to  plow  ahead  and  do 
what  is  needed. 

Usually  it  is  extremely  difficult  or  impossible  to  learn  whether  the 
abnormal  behavior  of  these  young  children  had  its  roots  in  organic  trouble 
or  in  childhood  experiences  or,  as  the  psychiatrists  and  psychologists  at  the 
Center  are  inclined  to  think,  in  both. 

As  an  example  of  the  difficulty  in  tracing  the  origin  of  abnormal 
behaviors,  Dr.  Churchill  tells  about  S-year-old  Jimmy  who  socks  himself 
on  the  head  and  jaw  until  he  is  bruised  and  his  knuckles  have  calluses. 

425 


During  some  20-minute  periods  he  has  been  observed  to  hit  himself  more 
than  2,000  times. 

After  studying  Jimmy's  history,  the  psychiatrist  first  thought  that  the 
behavior  probably  had  been  learned,  in  the  sense  that  Jimmy  had  received 
a  lot  of  attention  for  it.  Adults  would  step  in  and  hold  his  hands.  Jimmy 
would  hit  himself  while  eating,  too,  so  his  parents  would  hold  his  hands 
and  feed  him.  Things  got  to  the  point  where  he  was  being  held  most  of  the 
time  and  was  refusing  to  feed  himself. 

Under  treatment  at  the  Center,  everything  that  might  be  considered  to 
reinforce  or  reward  the  hitting  behavior  was  withdrawn,  but  instead  of 
dying  out,  as  learning  theory  says  should  be  the  case,  the  behavior 
continued.  Theie  was  some  improvement,  in  that  Jimmy  began  feeding 
himself,  but  Dr.  Churchill  noticed  a  strange  correlation:  the  more  the  boy 
ate,  the  more  he  also  used  himself  as  a  punching  bag. 

Recently  the  staff  tried  a  different  approach— the  administration  of 
painful  but  harmless  electiic  shock,  paired  with  the  word  shock,  whenever 
Jimmy  began  hitting  himself.  Within  3  days  his  hitting  behavior,  which 
had  been  essentially  unaltered  for  3  years,  practically  stopped.  Further, 
the  investigator  reports,  this  behavior  can  now  be  controlled  by  the  word 
shock  alone.  The  boy  has  become  more  approachable  and  is  playing  with 
toys,  which  he  had  ignored  for  months. 

Some  physiological  factor  is  believed  to  be  at  least  partly  involved  in 
Jimmy's  tiouble, 

Sally,  another  child  at  the  Center,  typically  withdraws  to  a  corner  of 
the  playroom,  sits  on  the  floor,  and  rocks  endlessly.  All  children  do  a  little 
rocking,  Dr.  Churchill  notes:  they  find  pleasure  in  the  rhythm.  Why  some 
schizophrenic  children  keep  it  up  for  hours  is  not  known. 

In  this  connection,  Dr.  Gerald  D.  Alpern,  a  psychologist*  calls  iU- 
tention  to  blindisms,  the  term  applied  to  the  repetitive  behavior—such  as 
rocking,  swaying,  and  passing  the  hands  back  and  forth  in  front  of  the 
eyes— seen  in  many  blind  children.  With  much  of  their  external  slimuhi- 
tion  cut  off,  these  children  have  turned  to  themselves  for  stimulation. 
Blind  youngsters  are  not  mentally  ill,  but  they  do  obviously  have  im 
organic  defect:  they  cannot  see.  There  is  a  possibility,  then,  that  schizo- 
phrenic children  who  engage  in  behavior  similar  to  blindisms  also  have  un 
organic  defect.  There  is  also  a  possibility  that  blind  children  who  engage 
most  strongly  in  such  behavior  have  an  emotional  disturbance  as  well  as  itn 
organic  defect.  This  second  possibility  is  now  being  studied  at  the 
University  of  Indiana  Medical  School  as  part  of  a  different  research 
program. 


Some  of  the  Center's  Other  Research  Projects 

To  help  pin  down  the  factors  contributing  to  early  childhood  psychosis, 
Dr.  DeMyer  and  her  associates  are  comparing  in  great  detail  30  schizo- 
phrenic children  and  their  families  with  30  normal  children  and  their 
families.  The  children  are  given  physical  and  neurological  examinations 

426 


and  are  tested  psychologically.  Samples  of  blood  and  urine  are  analyzed 
for  the  presence  of  abnormal  metabolic  products.  Information  about  their 
behavior  and  growth  from  birth  onward— including  eating,  sleeping, 
physical  and  mental  development,  and  social  skills— and  about  the 
attitudes,  personalities,  and  life  histories  of  their  parents  is  obtained  from 
an  exhaustive  series  of  interviews,  covering  13  schedules,  with  the  fathers 
and  mothers. 

The  investigator  points  out  that  the  study,  like  other  research  on  the 
causes  of  schizophrenia,  looks  backward  and  therefore  has  to  rely  for 
much  of  its  information  on  what  the  parents  remember  and  choose  to  tell. 
However,  it  carefully  checks  the  account  of  one  parent  against  that  of  the 
other  and  it  carefully  compaies  all  the  data  about  a  family  in  which  a 
child  is  schizophrenic  with  all  the  data  about  a  family  that  is  similar  to  it 
except  for  the  absence  of  schizophrenia. 

Besides  having  to  make  its  observations  after  the  fact,  research  on  the 
causes  of  schizophrenia  is  frequently  hampered  also  by  the  fallibility  of 
clinical  judgment.  Drawing  upon  her  own  experience,  Dr.  DeMyer  says  an 
investigator  is  likely  to  get  quite  definite  impressions  about  the  first 
families  he  sees  in  a  study  like  this  and  then,  unless  he  is  very  careful,  he 
tends  to  see  the  rest  of  the  families  in  the  light  of  his  early  impressions. 
Dr.  DeMyer,  trying  to  be  very  careful,  found  that  the  more  schizophrenic 
children  she  studied,  the  wider  the  lange  of  characteristics  she  saw  in  their 
families. 

To  minimize  the  problem  of  clinical  judgment  and  to  permit  hundreds 
of  facts  about  each  family  to  be  considered,  all  data  in  the  current 
research  are  given  numerical  ratings,  and  the  comparisons  will  be  made  by 
a  computer. 

Tying  in  with  this  woik  on  the  antecedents  of  childhood  schizophrenia 
is  a  recently  completed  survey  of  the  families  of  99  schizophrenic  children 
and  146  disturbed  but  not  psychotic  children.  These  children  had  been 
seen  at  the  La  Rue  Carter  Hospital  between  1955  and  1 963,  and  the  data 
about  their  families  came  from  the  files.  The  parents  of  schizophrenic 
children,  no  matter  what  the  type  of  schizophrenia,  were  found  to  be 
significantly  better  educated  than  the  other  parents  (who  had  about  the 
same  amount  of  schooling  as  the  average  Indiana  adult)  and  the  fathers 
held  much  better  jobs. 

These  findings  (a)  support  the  earlier  observation  by  another  investigator 
that  parents  of  autistic  children  tend  to  be  well  educated  and  successful, 
and  (b)  extend  this  observation  to  include  the  parents  of  schizophrenic 
children  in  general.  Another  interesting  and  unexplained  finding:  among 
the  disturbed  but  nonpsychotic  children,  broken  homes  were  common; 
among  the  schizophrenic  children,  they  were  not. 

Other  research  underway  by  the  Indianapolis  group  includes: 
•    Observations  to  test  Dr.  Churchill's  hunch  that  autistic  children  at 
times  pay  considerably  more  attention  to  adults  when  the  adults 
seem  to  be  paying  no  attention  to  the  children.  In  terms  of  condi- 
tioning theory,  this  would  mean  that  the  attention  of  adults  had 

427 


become  negatively  reinforcing,  and  it  would  have  important  implica- 
tions for  therapy. 

A  variety  of  studies  intended  to  learn  more  about,  and  to  describe 
systematically,  the  behavior  of  schizophrenic  children.  In  one  of 
these,  a  moving  picture  is  made  of  each  child's  "behavior  clay"  by 
filming  him  for  10  seconds  eveiy  5  minutes,  from  the  time  he  wakes 
up  to  the  time  he  goes  to  sleep.  The  films  will  be  used  to  compare 
the  dominant  behavioral  characteristics  of  the  different  types  of 
schizophrenic  children  and  also  to  study  changes  in  the  children  from 
one  year  to  the  next.  Another  study  of  behavior  is  concerned  with 
the  way  autistic  children  use  toys.  The  first  findings  show  that 
autistic  children,  as  compared  with  groups  of  normal  and  retimltxl 
children,  (a)  make  fewer  different  uses  of  a  given  toy,  and  (b)  spend 
less  time  using  one  toy  in  combination  with  another. 
An  attempt  to  develop  a  quantitative  means  of  measuring  Uie 
abilities  of  young  schizophrenic  children  who  cannot  be  tested  on 
standard  intelligence  tests.  In  one  test  being  used  by  the  investigator, 
Dr.  Alpern,  the  youngster  is  scored  on  the  basis  of  his  failure  to  curry 
out  certain  activities  of  which  he  is  known  to  be  capable.  The  reason- 
ing here  is  that  it  may  be  less  important  to  know  what  a  schi/.o- 
phrenic  child  can  do  than  to  know  what  he  won't  do.  The  variability 
in  his  "failure  score"  over  time,  it  is  hoped,  may  be  a  useful  indicator 
of  how  well  he  will  respond  to  treatment. 


Research  Grant:  MH  5154 
Date  of  Interview:  Apr.  26,  1966 

References. 

DeMyer,  Marian  K.,  &  Ferster,  C.  B.  Teaching  new  social  behavior  to  schizophrenic 

children./.  Child Psychiat.,  1963,  1,  3 
Ferster,  C.  B.,  &  DeMyer,  Marian  K.  A  method  for  the  experimental  analysis  of  (he 

behavior  of  autistic  children,  Amer.  J.  Orthopsychiat.,  1962,  32,  1 
Hingtgen,  J.  N.,  Sanders,  Beverly  J.,  &  DeMyer,  Marian  K.  Shaping  cooperative 

responses  in  early  childhood  schizophrenics.  Presented  at  annual  meeting  of  the 

Amer.  Psychol,  Association,  1963. 
Lowe,   Lois  H.  Families  of  children  with  early  childhood  schizophrenia:  scluclcd 

demographic  information.  Arch.  Gen.  Psyclnal.,  1966,  14. 
Onheiber,  Phyllis,  White,  P.  T.,  DeMyer,  Marian  K.,  &  Ottinger,  D.  R.  Sleep  and  drcmu 

patterns  of  child  schizophrenics.  Arch.  Gen,  Psychiat.,  1965,  12. 
Tilton,  J.  R.,  &  Ottinger,  D.  R.  Comparison  of  the  toy  play  behavior  of  autistic, 

retarded,  and  normal  children.  Psychol  Rep,,  1964,  15. 
White,  P.  T.,  DeMyer,  W.,  &  DeMyer,  Marian  K.  EEC  abnormalities  in  early  childhood 

schizophrenia:   a   double-blind   study   of  psychiatrically   disturbed  and  normal 

children  during  promazine  sedation.  Amer.  J.  Psychiat.,  1964,  120,  10. 


Investigator 

Douglas  A.  Sargent,  M.D. 

The  Merrill-Palmer  Institute 

Detroit,  Mich. 

Prepared  by: 

Clarissa  Wittenberg 


Through  a  special  piogram  of  foster  home  placement,  a  number  of 
seriously  disturbed  boys  who  seemed  headed  foi  a  lifelong  series  of 
troubles  that  would  burden  the  community  as  well  as  themselves  appear 
to  have  been  helped  toward  a  relatively  normal  adolescence. 

This  program,  known  as  the  Detroit  Foster  Homes  Project,  so  far 
appeals  to  have  been  successful  even  though  the  boys,  neglected  by  their 
parents,  had  previously  undergone  at  least  two  and  as  many  as  nine  un- 
successful foster  home  placements.  The  community  agencies  that  referred 
them  to  the  project  considered  that  further  placement  in  the  usual  type  of 
foster  home  would  be  difficult  if  not  impossible. 

Persons  acquainted  with  the  neglected  childien  in  our  communities  find 
it  hard  to  be  optimistic  about  their  future.  But  the  Detroit  program 
suggests  that  some  of  the  most  troubled  and  troublesome  of  these  children 
may  be  put  on  the  path  to  a  normal  life. 

The  projects  which  was  sponsored  and  staffed  by  the  Merrill-Palmer 
Institute  of  Human  Development  and  Family  Life,  grew  out  of  concern 
for  the  boys  who  were  being  returned  to  the  care  of  the  Juvenile  Court 
because  they  were  deemed  no  longer  suitable  for  foster  care.  These 
children  weie  either  so  aggressive  or  disturbed  as  to  be  difficult  to  manage, 
yet  foster  care  was  considered  the  treatment  of  choice.  Until  these  boys 
liad  had  the  stabilizing  opportunity  to  live  in  one  place  with  people  who 
cared  about  them,  it  was  felt,  all  other  forms  of  assistance  would  be 
ineffectual. 

Fifteen  boys  between  the  ages  of  7  and  13  were  selected  to  test  two 
ideas:  (1)  That  the  "holding  power"  of  foster  homes  could  be  increased  to 
the  point  where  it  could  sustain  the  impact  of  such  problem  children  and 
thus  keep  them  from  detention  or  reform  facilities,  and  (2)  that  place- 
ment of  troubled  children  in  this  age  group  would  make  it  possible  for 
them  to  adjust  and  stabilize  before  the  turbulence  of  adolescence.  These 
boys,  1 1  of  whom  were  Negro  and  4  white,  had  histories  that  included 
chronic  anxiety,  school  failure  as  well  as  such  behavior  as  stealing, 
vandalism,  truancy,  setting  fires,  and  fighting. 

429 


Children  were  not  accepted  if  they  showed  signs  of  serious  physical 
illness,  brain  damage,  psychosis,  or  retardation.  One  of  the  boys  accepted, 
however,  was  suspected  of  being  psychotic  and  this  was  later  confirmed. 

Only  a  small  number  of  the  boys  had  had  a  few  as  two  past  placements, 
the  number  established  as  a  minimum  for  acceptance  by  the  project.  The 
average  number  of  "official"  placements  was  four.  Often  a  child  had  also 
been  in  numerous  unrecorded,  "temporary"  placements,  Besides  the 
placements,  many  of  the  children  early  in  life  had  been  shifted  from 
relative  to  relative.  In  short,  the  relationships  and  living  situations  of  all 
the  boys  had  been  continually  disrupted. 

One  boy,  for  example,  had  been  deseited  by  his  mother,  cared  for  by 
his  grandmother,  and  been  placed  in  two  temporary  homes,  all  by  the 
time  he  was  a  year  old.  He  remained  in  his  next  home  for  5  years.  It  was 
then  declared  unfit  and  he  was  moved.  His  consequent  unhappincss  and 
disorientation  made  themselves  appaient  in  school,  so  he  was  moved 
again,  this  time  to  let  him  "benefit"  from  a  better,  more  suppoitive  school 
program,  but  he  then  began  to  picsent  difficulties  in  his  foster  homo  as 
well,  so  he  was  taken  to  a  community  shelter  for  children.  There  he  wiis 
described  by  the  staff  psychiatrist  as  the  most  disturbed  child  he  had  ever 
seen.  He  was  moved  briefly  to  the  Youth  Home  of  the  Juvenile  Court 
because  his  behavior  was  uncontrollable  at  the  shelter.  From  there  he  was 
admitted  to  a  small  children's  unit  of  a  psychiatric  hospital.  After  rcniiiiii- 
ing  there  half  a  year,  he  was  referred  to  the  project.  During  his  first  visit  lo 
the  office  for  evaluation,  he  hid  under  benches  and  lockeis  and  refused  to 
speak.  He  attempted  to  frustrate  an  attempt  at  psychological  testing,  by 
trying  to  stick  his  hand  into  the  blades  of  a  fan.  Incidents  or  even  minor 
disappointments  often  set  off  tantrums  of  unreasoning  rage  which  lasted 
as  long  as  an  houi.  During  these  he  was  very  destructive  and  it  sometimes 
took  two  adults  to  restrain  him.  Later  on  he  killed  and  maimed  pets  En  Die 
foster  home. 

Another  boy  came  to  the  attention  of  the  Juvenile  Court  after  three 

siblings  were  hospitalized  for  tuberculosis  and  a  man  living  in  the  house 

with  the  children  and  their  mother  was  found  to  have  an  active  cast)  of  I  he 

disease.  When  the  juvenile  authorities  came  to  investigate,  the  mother 

could  not  be  found.  Subsequently  this  boy  maintained  that  if  his  mother 

had  known  they  were  going  to  come  that  day,  she  would  not  have  lei  Ihe 

police  take  him  and  that  she  would  try  to  get  him  back  as  soon  us  site 

could.  This  he  maintained  despite  the  fact  that  she  rarely  visited  him  tnuE 

made  no  attempt  to  remove  him  from  the  detention  home.  While  lie  was 

detained  there  he  fought  constantly.  Obscene  or  critical  comments  about 

mothers  weie  especially  likely  to  trigger  his  rage.  He  was  academically 

retarded.  He  couldn't  read.  His  school  record  showed  that  for  many  years 

HP  hnH  rtfto.i  ™«p  to  school  so  tired  and  hungry  he  could  not  study.  He 

:ause  he  felt  it  better  to  be  considered  bad  than  stupid. 

stories  were  similarly  dreary,  varying  only  in  detail.  In 

/orkers  had  tried  to  help  the  family  and  avoid   tlic 

of  these  attempts  were  ineffectual.  The  Juvenile  Court 

.  to  take  custody  of  the  child.  In  no  case  was  a  return  to 


the  parents  a  possible  alternative,  as  their  lives  were  too  chaotic.  Most  of 
the  parents  had  chopped  all  contact  with  the  boys.  One  mother  was  in  a 
mental  hospital  and  seveial  others  had  histories  of  hospitalization  for 
mental  illness  or  alcoholism.  All  of  the  families  were  broken  by  divorce, 
separation,  death,  or  the  fact  that  the  mother  had  never  been  married. 
One  father  was  in  prison.  None  of  the  fathers  visited  their  sons.  Several 
mothers  themselves  had  been'court  wards  as  children.  The  occasional  visits 
between  these  children  and  their  relatives  usually  were  followed  by  in- 
creased anxiety  or  distuibed  behavior  in  the  children. 

The  extent  of  family  breakdown  and  its  attendant  problems  was  in- 
creased by  the  fact  that  the  boys  often  came  from  large  families.  Ten  of 
these  families  had  from  7  to  14  children.  Each  child  with  siblings  had 
siblings  in  foster  care.  Rarely  had  any  been  placed  together.  Only  six  of 
the  boys  ever  had  been  able  to  maintain  contact  with  their  siblings. 


The  Foster  Parents 

In  spite  of  an  intensive  campaign  and  the  full  cooperation  of  other 
agencies,  the  project  found  it  difficult  to  find  foster  parents  who  met  even 
Its  rathei  permissive  standards.  These  required  that  the  foster  patents  be 
self-supporting,  without  obvious  mental  disorder,  be  able  to  read  and  to 
value  education.  Further  they  were  to  have  some  emotional  flexibility  and 
the  capacity  to  cooperate  with  professional  workers.  (This  last  point 
turned  out  to  be  of  paramount  importance.)  As  it  turned  out,  applicants 
were  eliminated  only  for  such  serious  reasons  as  psychosis  or  marginal 
financial  01  occupational  status.  Only  5  percent  of  applicants  were  final- 
ly accepted. 

In  the  end,  the  project  reports,  it  had  an  acceptable  group  of  foster 
parents,  seriously  interested  for  the  most  part  in  what  it  was  trying  to  do 
but  more  like  the  average  foster  parent  than  the  ideal— and  better  so,  since 
this  was  a  demonstration  program.  A  few  of  the  foster  parents  turned  out 
to  have  rather  serious  emotional  problems  which  only  became  apparent  as 
involvement  with  the  child  deepened. 

The  foster  parents  ranged  in  age  from  30  to  64,  with  the  average  father 
being  49  and  the  average  mother  44.  Ten  couples  had  had  only  one 
marriage.  Eight  had  been  married  21  years  or  more.  Ten  had  children  of 
their  own,  and  eight  of  these  couples  had  children  still  living  at  home.  Ten 
of  the  foster  parents  came  from  the  South,  the  others  from  the  urban 
North,  Four  had  been  foster  children  themselves. 

Ten  of  the  families  were  buying  their  homes,  and  two  owned  their 
btisinesses.  One  father  was  a  professional  engineer,  and  the  rest  had  had 
long,  steady  employment  in  various  Detroit  factories.  Several  of  the 
women  had  had  training  in  practical  nursing,  beauty  parlor  work,  office 
machine  operation.  Several  had  held  long-term  domestic  positions.  One 
had  worked  in  a  factory.  Family  incomes  for  the  most  part  were  in  the 
vicinity  of  $6,000. 

431 


Most  of  the  foster  patents  had  had  considerably  less  than  a  high  school 
education,  a  finding  that  agrees  with  the  results  of  a  preliminary  study,  by 
the  project  directors,  showing  a  low  state  of  literacy  to  be  common  among 
foster  parents  in  general.  Because  of  this  low-educational  level>  project 
workers  used  little  written  material  and  routinely  offered  assistance  wlieu- 
ever  forms  had  to  be  filled  out.  This  lack  of  education  became  a  more 
obvious  handicap  when  the  parents  tried  to  help  the  child  with  school- 
work  or  to  communicate  with  his  teacher. 

A  high  degree  of  community  responsibility  existed  among  the  foster 
parents.  Several  were  active  in  their  churches,  unions,  or  neighborhood 
groups.  Most  of  them  were  quite  involved  with  their  relatives  and  spent  a 
great  deal  of  time  with  them. 

These  people  gave  a  variety  of  reasons  for  wanting  to  be  foster  parents. 
Several  childless  couples  said  that  with  a  foster  child  they  could  feel  they 
had  a  family.  A  few  other  couples  said  they  had  worked  hard,  had 
achieved  substantial  income  and  comfort,  and  now  wanted  to  do  some- 
thing for  an  unfortunate  child.  Several  wanted  companions  for  their  own 
children.  Some  had  previously  been  foster  parents  and  had  found  it  a 
satisfying  experience.  Several  foster  mothers  hoped  to  repeat  the  highly 
gratifying  experience  they  had  had  in  caring  for  their  own  children.  In  itt 
least  one  case  the  foster  mother  was  lonely  and  wanted  company.  At  leiist 
one  couple  hoped  ultimately  to  adopt  the  foster  child.  Subsequently  it 
became  evident  that  in  addition  to  these  overt  expectations,  there  were 
other  needs  which  led  these  couples  into  foster  care  and  not  all  of  these 
needs  were  compatible  with  the  job,  as  it  turned  out. 


Working  With  the  Foster  Parents:  the  First  Steps 

Through  frequent,  sometimes  daily  discussions  with  individual  pjirunts 
or  couples  and  through  group  discussions,  both  before  and  after  the  hoys 
had  been  placed,  the  staff  tried  to  help  the  parents  understand  and  cope 
with  the  boys'  behavior. 

Major  attention  was  given  to  widening  the  foster  parents'  range  of 
disciplinary  techniques.  Because  many  of  the  boys  had  suffered  physical 
abuse  and  been  emotionally  hurt  by  it,  spanking  was  specifically 
prohibited  and  help  given  the  parents  in  developing  alternative  ways  of 
dealing  with  undesirable  behavior,  Particularly  encouraged  were  (ho 
anticipation  of  explosive  situations,  the  avoidance  of  overstimulation,  and 
the  use  of  words  rather  than  blows  as  a  means  of  communication. 

Many  of  the  foster  parents  had  had  an  abiding  faith  in  the  efficacy  of 
spanking  in  raising  their  own  children.  The  project's  rule  against  it  created 
and  was  not  especially  successful.  Spanking  occurred,  the  project 
Tn  school,  too,  spanking  or  slapping  was  sometimes  resorted  lo  in 
-3  project's  plea.  They  conclude  that,  for  a  variety  of  reasons, 
sonditions  of  the  project,  the  occasional  use  of  physical  punish- 
levitable. 


The  project  attempted  to  place  the  children  so  that  each  would  be  the 
youngest  child  in  the  family  to  lessen  the  competition  with  the  family's 
own  children. 

Before  a  boy  was  actually  placed,  a  caseworker  attempted  to  build  a 
good  relationship  both  with  him  and  with  the  foster  family.  Great  pains 
were  taken  to  pace  the  transition  from  detention  home,  etc.,  to  the  new 
foster  home  according  to  the  needs  of  the  child  and  foster  family.  Then, 
for  many  days  after  placement,  the  caseworker  was  available  almost 
continually.  This  was  important,  the  project  found,  because  the  parents 
and  the  child  tended  to  have  a  great  many  questions  and  worries  about 
each  other  and  to  be  reluctant  to  deal  with  them.  Many  of  these  worries 
were  acted  out  directly  by  the  children,  and  the  casewoiker's  help  was 
needed  to  cope  with  these  reactions. 

In  some  of  the  first  cases,  the  parents  had  parties  for  the  children  when 
they  arrived.  But  some  foster  children,  it  developed,  were  under  so  much 
strain  that  they  could  not  stand  this  welcoming  ceremony  and  ran  away. 
So  in  the  latei  cases,  on  the  advice  of  the  staff,  the  family's  response  to  the 
arrival  of  the  foster  child  was  modified  and  played  down. 

In  general,  the  best  beginnings  were  in  those  families  that  deviated  as 
little  as  possible  from  the  noimal  routine  and  had  other  children  to  help 
ease  the  strain  of  fitting  into  the  new  neighborhood. 

All  the  boys  tested  the  parents  to  see  if  and  when  they  would  be 
rejected.  The  hardest  kind  of  behavior  for  some  of  the  parents  to  deal 
with  was  withdrawal.  Some  of  the  boys  became  almost  mute,  and 
physically  unavailable  to  their  new  parents.  In  other  cases,  oaths  and 
obscenities  were  used  to  test  foster  parents  or  to  keep  them  at  a  distance. 

Some  of  the  foster  parents  asked  that  the  boy  be  removed  from  their 
care.  These  parents,  in  general,  felt  less  well  prepared  for  the  child  and 
looked  upon  the  caseworker  as  an  investigator  checking  up  on  them.  The 
other  parents  tended  to  see  him  as  a  helper  and  themselves  as  part  of  a 
team. 

The  parents  who  gave  up  the  children  seemed  to  have  been  most  upset 
by  aggression  directed  against  themselves  or  other  family  members.  They 
mentioned,  for  example,  "sassing  me,"  "fighting  with  my  son/* 
"defiance,"  and  "cursing  and  temper." 

The  parents  who  kept  children,  on  the  othei  hand,  seemed  to  be  more 
worded  about  the  child's  well-being.  Typically  their  concerns  focused  on 
such  activities  as  "staying  out  all  night,"  "backing  car  into  the  street 
(7-year-old),"  "walks  to  swimming  pool  by  himself.'*  It  may  be  that  the 
two  groups  of  parents  were  not  exposed  to  the  same  kinds  of  behavior. 
Temper  tantrums  were  reported  by  most  of  the  parents  who  gave  up 
children  but  by  only  a  few  of  the  other  parents. 


Approaches  and  Techniques 

In  spite  of  the  insulting  and  sometimes  assaultive  behavior  exhibited  by 
many  of  the  boys,  the  project  staff  made  the  continuing  stability  of  the 

433 


Most  of  the  foster  patents  had  had  considerably  less  than  a  high  school 
education,  a  finding  that  agrees  with  the  results  of  a  preliminary  study,  by 
the  project  directors,  showing  a  low  state  of  literacy  to  be  common  among 
foster  parents  in  general.  Because  of  this  low-educational  level,  project 
workers  used  little  written  material  and  routinely  offered  assistance  when- 
ever forms  had  to  be  filled  out.  This  lack  of  education  became  a  moie 
obvious  handicap  when  the  parents  tried  to  help  the  child  with  school- 
work  or  to  communicate  with  his  teacher. 

A  high  degiee  of  community  responsibility  existed  among  the  foster 
parents.  Several  were  active  in  their  churches,  unions,  or  neighborhood 
groups.  Most  of  them  were  quite  involved  with  their  relatives  and  spent  a 
great  deal  of  time  with  them. 

These  people  gave  a  variety  of  reasons  for  wanting  to  be  foster  parents. 
Several  childless  couples  said  that  with  a  foster  child  they  could  feel  they 
had  a  family.  A  few  other  couples  said  they  had  worked  hard,  luui 
achieved  substantial  income  and  comfort,  and  now  wanted  to  do  some- 
thing for  an  unfortunate  child.  Several  wanted  companions  for  their  own 
children.  Some  had  previously  been  foster  parents  and  had  found  it  a 
satisfying  experience.  Several  foster  mothers  hoped  to  repeat  the  highly 
gratifying  experience  they  had  had  in  caring  for  their  own  children.  In  ut 
least  one  case  the  foster  mother  was  lonely  and  wanted  company.  At  least 
one  couple  hoped  ultimately  to  adopt  the  foster  child.  Subsequently  it 
became  evident  that  in  addition  to  these  overt  expectations,  there  were 
other  needs  which  led  these  couples  into  foster  care  and  not  all  of  these 
needs  were  compatible  with  the  job,  as  it  turned  out. 


Working  With  the  Foster  Parents:  the  First  Steps 

Through  frequent,  sometimes  daily  discussions  with  individual  pnrcnts 
or  couples  and  through  group  discussions,  both  before  and  after  the  boys 
had  been  placed,  the  staff  tried  to  help  the  parents  understand  and  cope 
with  the  boys'  behavior. 

Major  attention  was  given  to  widening  the  foster  parents'  range  of 
disciplinary  techniques.  Because  many  of  the  boys  had  suffered  physical 
abuse  and  been  emotionally  hurt  by  it,  spanking  was  specifically 
prohibited  and  help  given  the  parents  in  developing  alternative  ways  of 
dealing  with  undesirable  behavior.  Particularly  encouraged  were  the 
anticipation  of  explosive  situations,  the  avoidance  of  overstimnlation,  njul 
the  use  of  words  rather  than  blows  as  a  means  of  communication. 

Many  of  the  foster  parents  had  had  an  abiding  faith  in  the  efficacy  of 

""""uing  in  raising  their  own  children.  The  project's  rule  against  it  crcntcd 

^  and  was  not  especially  successful.  Spanking  occurred,  the  project 

chool,  too,  spanking  or  slapping  was  sometimes  resorted  lo  In 

project's  plea.  They  conclude  that,  for  a  variety  of  reasons, 

jnditions  of  the  project,  the  occasional  use  of  physical  punish- 

evitable. 


The  project  attempted  to  place  the  children  so  that  each  would  be  the 
youngest  child  in  the  family  to  lessen  the  competition  with  the  family's 
own  children. 

Before  a  boy  was  actually  placed,  a  caseworker  attempted  to  build  a 
good  relationship  both  with  him  and  with  the  foster  family.  Great  pains 
were  taken  to  pace  the  transition  from  detention  home,  etc.,  to  the  new 
foster  home  according  to  the  needs  of  the  child  and  foster  family.  Then, 
for  many  days  after  placement,  the  caseworker  was  available  almost 
continually.  This  was  important,  the  project  found,  because  the  parents 
and  the  child  tended  to  have  a  great  many  questions  and  worries  about 
each  othei  and  to  be  reluctant  to  deal  with  them.  Many  of  these  worries 
were  acted  out  directly  by  the  children,  and  the  caseworker's  help  was 
needed  to  cope  with  these  reactions. 

In  some  of  the  fiist  cases,  the  parents  had  parties  for  the  children  when 
they  arrived.  But  some  foster  children,  it  developed,  were  under  so  much 
strain  that  they  could  not  stand  this  welcoming  ceremony  and  ran  away. 
So  in  the  later  cases,  on  the  advice  of  the  staff,  the  family's  response  to  the 
anival  of  the  foster  child  was  modified  and  played  down. 

In  general,  the  best  beginnings  were  in  those  families  that  deviated  as 
little  as  possible  from  the  noimal  routine  and  had  other  children  to  help 
ease  the  strain  of  fitting  into  the  new  neighborhood. 

All  the  boys  tested  the  parents  to  see  if  and  when  they  would  be 
rejected.  The  hardest  kind  of  behavior  for  some  of  the  parents  to  deal 
with  was  withdrawal.  Some  of  the  boys  became  almost  mute,  and 
physically  unavailable  to  their  new  parents.  In  other  cases,  oaths  and 
obscenities  were  used  to  test  foster  parents  or  to  keep  them  at  a  distance. 

Some  of  the  foster  parents  asked  that  the  boy  be  removed  from  their 
care.  These  patents,  in  general,  felt  less  well  prepared  for  the  child  and 
looked  upon  the  caseworker  as  an  investigator  checking  up  on  them.  The 
other  parents  tended  to  see  him  as  a  helper  and  themselves  as  part  of  a 
team, 

The  parents  who  gave  up  the  children  seemed  to  have  been  most  upset 
by  aggression  directed  against  themselves  or  other  family  members.  They 
mentioned,  for  example,  "sassing  me,"  "fighting  with  my  son," 
"defiance,**  and  "cursing  and  temper." 

The  parents  who  kept  children,  on  the  other  hand,  seemed  to  be  more 
worried  about  the  child's  well-being.  Typically  their  concerns  focused  on 
such  activities  as  "staying  out  all  night,**  "backing  car  into  the  street 
(7-year-old),"  "walks  to  swimming  pool  by  himself."  It  may  be  that  the 
two  groups  of  parents  were  not  exposed  to  the  same  kinds  of  behavior. 
Temper  tantrums  were  reported  by  most  of  the  parents  who  gave  up 
children  but  by  only  a  few  of  the  other  parents. 


Approaches  and  Techniques 

In  spite  of  the  insulting  and  sometimes  assaultive  behavior  exhibited  by 
many  of  the  boys,  the  project  staff  made  the  continuing  stability  of  the 

433 


foster  home  one  of  its  key  objectives.  Trying  to  hold  to  this  objective 
became  very  difficult  at  times.  For  example,  one  child's  behavior  was 
occasionally  so  disturbed-it  reached  a  peak  when  he  killed  a  neighbor- 
hood puppy-that  maintaining  the  placement  was,  to  some  degree,  a 
threat  to  the  community. 

The  easiest  action  in  this  case,  the  project  reports,  would  have  been  to 
remove  the  child.  This  was  avoided,  the  crisis  passed,  and  the  placement 
has  been  sustained.  There  is  no  certainty  that  he  will  not  eventually  be 
hospitalized  anyway;  as  the  fostei  mother  put  it,  however,  "If  he  doesn't 
make  it  here,  he'll  never  make  it." 

A  foster  home  placement,  the  staff  points  out,  demands  a  great  deal  of 
work  and  devotion  from  the  foster  patents  and  a  great  deal  from  the 
foster  child  as  well.  The  child,  however,  cannot  begin  attempting  to 
modify  his  own  behavior  until  he  has  some  certainty  that  people  aic  going 
to  support  him  and  to  continue  to  be  concerned  about  him.  Boys  like 
those  in  the  pioject  cannot  be  expected  to  increase  their  trust  until  they 
become  involved  with  people  who  aie  tiustworthy. 

The  staff  iccognized,  and  tried  to  get  foster  paients  and  teacheis  to 
recognize,  that  the  boys  had  suffered  real  deprivation.  The  aim  was  to 
repair  the  effects  of  this,  to  meet  the  child's  needs  at  all  levels,  and  at  tlie 
same  time  to  make  appropriate  demands  on  the  child. 

Each  foster  home  had  a  variety  of  cases— among  them  illness,  death, 
financial  difficulty,  the  mother's  going  out  to  work,  and  the  threatened 
separation  of  the  parents— that  endangered  its  stability.  But  the  crises  were 
often  temporaiy  or,  in  the  judgment  of  the  project,  not  so  harmful  to  the 
child  as  moving  him  would  have  been.  The  policy  was  to  scrutinize  the 
health  of  the  home  and  its  membeis  continually  but  not  to  move  a  child 
without  exceptional  reasons.  There  is  some  evidence  to  suggest  that  the 
involvement  of  the  foster  child  in  coping  with  these  crises  was  beneficial 
to  him. 

In  its  work  with  the  children  and  their  foster  parents,  the  project  used 
numerous  approaches  and  techniques,  the  main  ones  summarized  below. 

Casework  in  the  home  itself  was  emphasized. -To  answer  questions  us 
they  came  up,  and  to  give  all  the  assistance  possible,  the  caseworker 
visited  the  foster  parents  and  the  child  at  least  once  a  week— more 
frequently  if  the  situation  wairanted.  Even  when  the  parents  icporlcc) 
having  no  particular  problems  or  nothing  to  discuss,  the  visits  were  made 
and-often  these  were  the  most  fruitful  ones. 

Originally  it  had  been  planned  to  have  the  foster  parents  meet  regularly 

as  a  group,  and  this  was  to  have  become  the  key  educational  medium. 

However,  most  parents  showed  little  interest  in  the  planned  programs  anil 

attended  only  sporadically.  The  only  programs  that  were  well  attended 

were  those  planned  to  recognize  the  work  of  the  parents,  such  as  riu: 

annual  Hinnpr  Nnr  did  a  group  session  turn  out  to  be  a  place  to  shait 

arents,  by  and  large,  considered  these  difficulties  tc 

-nd  were  often  reluctant  to  discuss  them,  even  witl 


Psychiatric  and  other  professional  help  was  always  available.—  Periodic 
psychiatric  interviews  were  held  with  the  boys.  Psychiatric  consultation 
was  always  available  to  the  caseworkers,  whenever  an  emergency  seemed 
to  warrant  it.  Other  specialists  in  appropriate  fields,  such  as  education  and 
psychology,  were  project  consultants  and  were  called  upon  for  help  as 
needed.  Beyond  this,  the  actual  progress  of  the  project  was  kept  under 
continued  surveillance  and  modification  by  the  administrative  staff  and 
consultants. 

Group  work  services  were  used  for  the  boys,  the  general  objective  of 
which  was  to  help  a  boy  fit  into  his  new  home  and  neighborhood.  Specific 
aims  were  to  give  him  healthy  experiences  with  other  children,  help  him 
attain  social  skills,  especially  to  Icain  how  to  play. 

In  the  beginning  boys  were  added  to  the  group  as  they  were  accepted 
by   the   project,   but   the   group   became  unwieldy.  To  accomplish  the 
therapeutic  aims  of  the  group,  these  boys  needed  extensive  assistance  and 
new  caiefully  selected  groups  were  formed  of  thiee  or  four  boys  each. 

Where  theie  was  another  young  child  in  the  foster  home,  he  was  included 
in  the  group.  Neighborhood  friends  weie  invited  to  share  in  the  group 
activities  which  included  games,  trips,  and  other  recreational  activities. 
Many  of  the  boys,  it  turned  out,  had  never  been  to  even  a  store,  a  bank,  or 
a  post  office.  Several  had  never  been  to  the  city  park,  Belle  Isle,  to  which 
the  entire  city  seems  to  flock  in  hot  weather.  Such  trips  were  also  used  to 
observe  and  modify  the  boy's  behavioral  troubles. 

Special  emphasis  was  given  to  schooling,— Because  parents  were  not  able 
to  keep  a  child  who  failed  continually  in  school,  and  because  these 
children  were  likely  to  do  so,  the  project  staff—along  with  the  foster 
parents,  if  possible-met  with  the  school's  administratois  and  teacheis  and 
discussed  the  boy's  problems  before  a  boy  was  enrolled  in  a  school.  It  was 
found  that  this  procedure  made  for  a  better  relationship  between  school 
and  paients  and  helped  to  save  the  foster  parent  from  being  criticized  for 
the  child's  behavior  or  conditions  which  had  developed  before  he  came  to 
their  home.  Discussions  about  the  boy's  needs  and  the  project's  ideas 
about  discipline  were  held  with  each  teacher. 

The  staff  kept  in  close  touch  with  each  school  and  was  ready  to  help 
whenever  difficulties  arose.  When  a  school  called  for  assistance,  members 
of  the  staff  responded  at  once.  Consultants  were  brought  in  when 
advisable.  Personal  history  data  and  diagnoses  of  the  children  were  shared 
with  each  teacher  whenever  the  project  judged  this  information  to  be 
useful.  Most  of  the  time  this  confidence  was  justified.  Sometimes  it 
backfired. 

The  staff  found  that  the  boys  tended  to  do  better  in  highly  structured 
situations,  such  as  a  class  in  arithmetic,  than  in  looser,  more  informal 
groupings  such  as  music  classes  and  library  and  gym  sessions.  Consequent- 
ly, when  a  boy  needed  a  shorter  school  day  in  order  to  survive  the  strain, 
these  latter  were  the  first  classes  eliminated. 

435 


Efforts  were  made  to  have  the  boys  spend  as  much  time  as  possible 
with  those  teachers  who  were  found  to  be  making  outstanding  contri- 
butions. Letters  of  appreciation  were  written  to  teachers  and  administra- 
tors. With  the  project's  coopeiation,  several  of  the  teachers  who  wert 
attending  university  classes  wrote  reports  of  their  work  with  the  boys. 

Tutoring  became  an  important  part  of  the  program.-^  commor 
problem-arid  one  contributing  to  behavior  difficulties  in  school-wa: 
reading  retardation.  Even  in  the  third  and  fourth  grades,  some  of  the  boyi 
were  virtually  nonreaders.  For  all  the  children  having  trouble  in  reading 
the  pioject  established  a  therapeutic  tutoring  program. 

The  tutors  set  out  to  reach  the  boys  by  using  material  familiar  to  them 
including  the  songs  of  the  Beatles  and  books  of  comics.  After  such  ,11 
introduction,  it  was  easier  to  get  on  with  conventional  reading  material 
The  sessions  were  planned  to  use  to  the  utmost  the  child's  ability  t< 
concentrate  and  comprehend  yet  keep  fatigue  to  a  minimum.  Food  nm 
other  treats  were  a  valuable  entree  into  the  confidence  of  these  deprive* 
boys. 

Reading  became  a  very  important  area  of  achievement,  the  projcc 
reports.  Often  the  boys  would  want  to  demonstrate  their  new  road  in 
ability  to  the  caseworker  and  other  adults. 

Incidentally,  the  tutoring  sessions  were  considered  a  valuable  means  c 
helping  to  meet  the  difficulties  brought  on  by  the  summertime  closiu 
of  the  schools.  Foi  some  of  these  boys,  June  meant  still  anolhc 
sepaiation  from  people  who  had  become  important  to  them,  and  face 
them  with  the  rather  aimless  and  formless-and  therefore  difficult  time  c 
summer.  Of  course,  some  of  the  boys  simply  were  glad  to  get  away  froi 
school. 

Efforts  were  made  to  strengthen  the  boy's  sense  of  identity, -T\\u  ii 
formation  available  about  his  background  was  given  to  the  foster  parent 
and  the  healthy  ties  remaining  from  his  past  were  nurtured.  Altonipi 
were  made  to  fill  in  the  voids  in  the  child's  knowledge  of  his  own  past. 

The  project  staff  initiated  contact  with  the  natural  parents  when  the 
could  be  located  and,  in  the  beginning,  in  some  cases,  arranged  for  thej 
to  visit  the  boys.  These  visits,  it  was  found,  always  caused  serioi 
difficulty  for  the  boys  and  in  some  cases  jeopardized  the  foster  hon 
placements.  Since  it  was  impossible  for  these  boys  to  return  to  the 
natural  parents,  and  since  the  foster  homes  were  considered  essential 
their  healthy  development,  parental  visits  came  to  be  virtually  eliminalc 
By  and  large,  the  staff  found,  the  situation  that  had  led  to  parental  ncglc 
or  abuse,  and  to  court  action  on  behalf  of  these  boys  had  tended 
deteriorate  rather  than  improve. 

wnc  a  relative  who  visited  and  maintained 
)oy.  Visits  in  such  cases  were  encourage 
ilped  a  boy  to  visit  a  past  foster  parent. 
sd  routinely  by  the  psychiatrist,  who  us 
ing  technique.  The  boys  were  fascinated  1 
;s-sometimes  they  were  taken  alone  a 
their  foster  family.  The  pictures  becat 


valued  possessions.  The  pictures  were  also  used  as  tangible  evidence  of  the 
project's  concern  for  the  child,  a  concrete  demonstration  that  his  person 
was  valued  (the  photos  decorated  the  project's  walls).  It  also  seemed  to 
enhance  the  child's  self-concept. 

Anothei  identity  strengthening  technique  was  used  in  the  tutoring 
sessions,  though  primarily  tp  provide  the  child  with  material  he  would  like 
to  read.  The  tutor  would  help  the  child  compose  a  story  about  himself 
and  then  would  have  it  typed  for  him. 


The  Outcome  of  the  Program 

The  project  repoits  the  following  results: 

1.  All  the  boys  have  received  good  medical  and  dental  care  and  are 
physically  well. 

2.  Thirteen  out  of  fifteen  boys  remain  either  in  foster  homes  or  have 
been  adopted.  Some  are  enjoying  a  high  degree  of  security  for  the  first 
time  in  their  lives. 

3.  Enuresis,  existing  in  one-half  of  the  boys,  has  ceased  in  every  case. 

4.  They  are  less  depressed  than  they  were. 

5.  Their  relationships  with  people  are  becoming  deeper  and  more  ap- 
propriate, although  there  is  still  a  tendency  to  try  to  keep  from  getting 
involved. 

6.  Most  are  behaving  more  nearly  at  a  level  appropriate  to  their  age.  In 
the  beginning,  many  had  a  tendency  to  play  with  children  much  younger 
than  themselves- frequently  to  the  despair  of  the   foster  parents.  One 
father,   for  instance,  who  had  hoped  for  a  companionable  youngster, 
found  that  the  boy  liked  most  of  all  to  ride  a  tricycle  or  play  with  the 
neighborhood  small  fry.  In  some  cases  there  are  still  remnants  of  such 
behavior. 

7.  Fears  and  anxiety,  both  conscious  and  as  revealed  in  the  dreams  and 
fantasies  of  the  boys,  are  less.  Those  remaining  are  more  realistic.  In  the 
beginning,  in  many   cases,  violent  scenes— peopled  with  monsters,  dis- 
membered peisons,  and  lost  children-predominated.  Fears  of  personal  or 
family  destruction  were  common, 

8.  The  boys'  attitude  toward  food  has  improved.  It  is  less  frequently 
used  to  assuage  anxiety  or  as  a  substitute  for  love.  Early  in  the  project, 
one  boy  who  was  offered  a  piece  of  cake  ate  the  whole  cake,  and  children 
offered  snacks  during  a  group  program  or  a  casework  session  frequently 
demanded  an  entire  meal.  Another  boy,  fearful  that  his  foster  parents  did 
not  love  him,  feared  that  he  could  not  have  a  second  helping;  another 
took  literally  his  foster  mother's  remark,  "We  don't  have  a  thing  in  the 
house  to  eat,"  and  slipped  out  to  a  neighbor's.  Still  another  ate  voraciously 
and  became  very  fat.  Tills  boy  is  now  secure  enough  to  be  able  to  diet,  has 
lost  30  pounds,  and  is  proud  of  the  change  in  his  appearance. 

9.  Antisocial  behavior  has  decreased.  From  diffuse  outbreaks  against 
the  community,  such  as  throwing  bricks  through  a  school  window,  they 
have  shifted  to  more  localized  acts  with  a  more  immediate  bearing  upon 

437 


themselves,  such  a  truancy.  One  boy  has  stopped  stealing  even  though  he 
had  been  taught  to  steal  by  his  mother. 

10.  In  school,  some  of  the  boys  are  still  behind  the  grade  level  ap- 
propriate for  their  age  but  all  have  made  progress.  One  boy  had  to  be  sent 
to  a  special  school  but  the  others  are  in  regular  classes.  The  nonreadcrs 
now  read. 

11.  Perhaps  the   most  marked  gain  has  been  in  verbal  facility  and 
spontaneity.  This  improvement  is  attributed  mainly  to  the  increase  in 
personal  security,  the  lessening  of  hostility,  encouragement  of  the  ability 
to    use    words   lather    than    action    to    express    themselves,    and    the 
opportunity  to  talk  with  friendly  people. 

12.  The  boys  are  less  tense  and  better  able  to  concentrate  than  when 
they  first  came  into  the  project.  Then  they  were  inclined  to  be  reckless 
and  physically  overactive.  They  often  avoided  eye  contact  or  met  the  eyes 
defiantly.  They  either  slumped  in  chairs  or  were  unable  to  lemain  seated. 
Regular  interview  situations  were  difficult  foi  them. 

13.  Contrary  to   some   commonly  voiced   fears,  the  boys  have  not 
"infected"  other  children -either  those  in  the  foster  family  or  those  in  tlic 
neighborhood.  In  the  case  of  the  boy  who  killed  a  puppy,  his  sadistic 
behavior  with  animals  has  been  of  concern  to  the  neighbors,  but  Ihcii 
children,  instead  of  following  his  lead,  tend  to  reject  this  behavior  and 
thus  to  exert  a  helpful  influence  upon  him.  In  only  one  case  was  there  a 
serious  problem  between  the  foster  child  and  the  natural  child:   they 
fought  to  such  an  extent  that  the  mother  asked  to  have  the  foster  child 
removed.  She  was  concerned  also  that  this  boy's  stealing  would  set  a  bad 
example  for  her  son.  (This  is  the  same  parent  who  wanted  a  foster  child  us 
a  companion  for  her  son.  This  placement  was  an  error  in  judgment,  ac- 
cording to  the  project.) 

Still  piesent  in  each  of  these  children  are  the  tremendous  aftereffects  of 
early  deprivation  and  of  repeated  separation  from  parental  figures.  The 
children  in  varying  degrees  still  show  signs  of  hostility,  distrust,  ap- 
prehension, and  sadness.  Nor  has  the  program  been  successful  in  every 
case.  One  boy,  for  example,  was  sent  to  a  mental  hospital.  (Luler 
authorities  returned  him  to  his  natural  mother,  though  she  was  barely  able 
to  care  for  herself.)  One  is  in  a  boy's  home  described  as  "rather  pleasant"; 
a  third  is  in  a  treatment-oriented  detention  facility  described  as  "relatively 
good," 

Nevertheless,  most  of  these  "impossible  to  place"  youngsters  have 

become  far  better  able  to  manage  their  day-to-day  living  in  a  foster  home. 

-am  i-.nn  ^»ionstrated,  its  directors  submit,  that  many  of  Ihc 

rp  JK  a  means  of  helping  such  children  can  be 

of   comprehensive   services,    It    then 

tment  with  the  advantages  of  a  stable 

ome  from  living  in  a  community.  Thus 

ly  to  have  required  institutional  care 

y  and  needed  no  halfway  house  to  ac- 

,he  end  of  their  treatment, 


Research  Grant'   Rll-MH-1551 
Date  of  Interview ,  Apr.  6,  1966 

References: 

Ambinder,  W    J.  The  extent  of  successive  placements  among  boys  in  fostei  homes. 

Child  Welf.,  July  1965,397-398. 
Ambinder,  W.  J.,  &  Falik,  H.  The  behaviorally  disturbed  foster  child  in  school— a 

preliminary  report.  Unpublished  paper.  Detroit  Foster  Homes  Project,  1965. 
Ambinder,  W.  J.  &  Falik,  H  The  social  acceptance  of  the  behaviorally  disturbed  boy  in 

his  classroom  group.  Unpublished  paper.  J,  Sch.  Psychol 
James,  Adnenne  Differences  between  two  groups  of  foster  parents.  Paper  presented  at 

Second  Annual  Conf.  on  Foster  Care  for  Emotionally  Disturbed  Children,  Detroit 

Foster  Homes  Project,  February,  1965. 
James,   Adnenne.   Casework   with   emotionally    disturbed   children   in   foster  care. 

Presented  at  NCSW,  Chicago,  1966. 
Mahaffey,  Maryann.  Progress  report  of  an  experiment  in  foster  care  for  disturbed  boys, 

Paper  read  at  Ohio  Welf.  Conf.,  Cincinnati,  October,  1 965. 
Redl,  F.,  &  Wineman,  D,  The  aggressive  child.  Glencoe,  111.1  Fiee  Press,  1957. 
Sargent,  D.  A.,  &  Ambinder,  W.  J.  Foster  parents  techniques  of  management  of 

preadolescent  boys'  deviant  behavior.  Child  Welfare,  February,  1965,  90—94. 
Sargent,  D.  A.,  Ambinder,  W.  J.,  &  Fireman,  Laura.  Verbal  abilities  and  literacy  levels. 

required  of  foster  parents.  Child  Welfate,  December  1  963,  502-503, 
Sargent,  D.  A  ,  Ambinder,  W.  J,,  Fireman,  Laura,  &  Wineman,  D.  Role  phenomena  and 

foster  care  for  disturbed  children.  Amer.  J.  Orthopsychiat.,  1962,  32,  1, 
Shiefman,  Emma   The  Beatles?  Yeah!  Yeah!  Yeah!  The  Reading  Teacher,  October, 

1965. 
Shiefman,  Emma.  A  school  teacher  in  a  child  welfare  agency.  Children,.    May— June 

1966,  116-118. 


439 


Investigator. 

Abraham  WiklerM.D.* 
Addiction  Research  Center,  NIMH 
Lexington,  Ky. 

Prepared  by 

Antoinette  A.  Gatozzi 

The  postadclict  patient  leaves  the  Lexington  hospital  free  of  drugs.  He 
has  had  the  best  tieatment  that  medicine  knows  how  to  provide.  Whether 
he  entered  as  a  Federal  prisoner  or  a  voluntary  patient,  he  was  carefully 
and  humanely  withdrawn  from  his  drug,  usually  heroin,  and  received 
expert  aftercaie  and  rehabilitation  services.  But  the  grim  fact  is  that  the 
discharged  postaddict  cannot  be  given  much  chance  to  lemain  free  of 
drugs.  One  of  three  patients  is  readmitted  because  he  has  relapsed  to  the 
use  of  narcotics. 

This  calculation  is  based  on  patient  records  of  the  two  U.S.  Public 
Health  Service  hospitals,  at  Fort  Worth,  Tex.,  and  Lexington,  Ky.,  that  arc 
charged  by  law  to  care  for  narcotic  addicts  in  the  United  States.  Patients 
are  Federal  prisoners  addicted  to  narcotic  drugs  and  narcotic  addicls  who 
voluntarily  enter  for  treatment.  Because  of  the  inherent  selectivity  of  this 
group,  the  readmission  late  would  seem  to  represent  a  lower  instance  of 
relapse  after  cure  than  that  which  prevails  among  the  postaddict  popula- 
tion as  a  whole.  A  certain  number  of  relapsed  postaddict  patients  defy 
accountability  as  such. 

Tragically,  some  shoot  themselves  with  a  fatal  overdose  of  opiiilcs, 
unknowingly  or  perhaps  suicidally  oblivious  of  their  lost  tolerance  to 
dosages  formerly  self-administered.  Doubtless  there  are  others  who  lake 
up  using  opiates  again,  stay  clear  of  the  law,  and  do  not  choose  to  be 
withdrawn.  Many  postaddicts  turn  to  abuse  of  different  drugs,  notably 
barbiturates.  A  study  made  2  years  ago  of  1 ,000  consecutive  admissions  to 
Lexington  found  one  fourth  of  the  patients  physically  dependent  on 
barbiturates  as  well  as  addicted  to  a  narcotic  drug.  Although  it  is  difficult 
to  discover  the  true  number  of  postaddicts  who  relapse  after  cure,  all 
observers  agree  that  the  rate  is  high  and  the  problem  pressing. 

At  the  Addiction  Research  Center,  a  facility  of  the  intramural  program 
of  the  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health,  the  question  of  relapse  is  one 
of  many  that  researchers  are  investigating.  Understanding  why  postiulditHs 
relapse  after  cure  depends  upon  insight  into  the  factors  involved  in  the 


*Now  at  the  school  of  Medicine,  University  of  Kentucky  Research  Foundation, 
Lexington,  Kentucky. 


initial  use  and  abuse  of  drugs.  More  information  must  be  gained  about  the 
mechanisms  of  tolerance,  dependence,  and  the  stereotyped  withdrawal 
syndrome  that  follows  abrupt  discontinuation  of  addicting  compounds 
befoie  the  question  of  relapse  can  be  answered  definitively. 

The  classic  drugs  of  addiction  are  the  opiates.  Of  these,  people  most 
often  hear  of  morphine  and  heroin.  Morphine  is  of  great  value  in  the 
practice  of  medicine;  its  prescriptive  use  by  physicians  is  controlled  by 
law.  Heroin  is  almost  identical  to  morphine  in  biological  activity.  Grain 
for  gram,  it  is  two  to  three  times  as  potent  as  morphine,  which  may 
represent  a  convenient  economy  to  those  who  deal  criminally  in  opiates 
and  thereby  explain  why  heroin  is  the  opiate  of  the  majoiity  of  narcotic 
addicts.  All  heroin  is  illegal  in  the  United  States.  An  opiate  interacts  with 
the  cells  of  an  individual's  somatic  and  autonomic  neivous  systems, 
profoundly  alters  the  functioning  of  vital  organs  and  glands,  and  affects 
his  mood  and  motivation.  Although  there  is  evidence  that  the 
phenomenon  begins  with  the  first  dose,  with  prolonged  use  the  individual 
becomes  physiologically  tolerant  to  the  drug's  actions,  and  the  original 
dose  no  longei  induces  the  original  effects.  A  very  small  proportion  of 
those  who  experience  the  effects  of  an  opiate  over  a  period  of  time 
become  dependent  on  the  drug.  They  feel  they  must  continue  using  it  in 
order  to  maintain  a  sense  of  well-being,  both  physical  and  mental. 

Why  Do  They  Relapse? 

Addicts  call  morphine  "God's  own  medicine."  Many  patients  with 
severe  pain  who  have  been  given  the  drug  by  their  physicians  would  agree 
with  that  assessment  because  the  sine  qua  noti  of  an  opiate  is  the  capacity 
to  relieve  pain.  No  other  compound  approaches  it  in  this  respect  although 
the  search  for  an  equally  effective  analgesic  with  lower  abuse  potentiality 
has  been  long  and  intense.  Opiates  also  possess  narcotic,  or  sleep- 
producing  properties,  and  for  this  reason  opium  derivatives  and  their 
synthetic  analogs  are  called  narcotic  analgesics.  Medical  scientists  do  not 
fully  undeistand  how  analgesics  (from  aspirin  to  morphine)  relieve  pain. 
There  is  still  much  to  learn  about  pain  itself  and  the  influence  of 
emotional  factors  on  the  threshold  level  at  which  noxious  physical  stimuli 
are  perceived  as  pain.  The  outstanding  capacity  of  the  opiates  to  relieve 
physical  pain  and  its  associative  psychic  components  is  probably  involved 
in  the  postaddict's  proclivity  to  relapse. 

Once  withdrawn  from  the  drug,  the  postaddict  faces  again  the  personal 
and  social  difficulties  that  formed  at  least  part  of  the  reason  why  he 
plunged  into  drug  abuse  initially.  The  narcotic  addict's  personal  problems 
are  thought  to  center  around  the  expression  of  aggression  and  the  primary 
drives  of  sexual  gratification  and  avoidance  of  pain.  These  normal  human 
drives  provoke  in  him  sharp,  sometimes  intolerable  anxieties,  and  he  may 
have  abused  the  drug  originally  because  it  led  him  to  a  state  of  dreamy 
indifference  to  reality.  The  social  factors  that  dispose  to  relapse  stem  from 
such  handicaps  as  a  truncated  education,  little  or  no  job  experience,  racial 

441 


minority-group  status,  and  a  criminal  record.  These  are  common  features 
of  the  backdrop  of  addiction.  All  aie  obstacles  to  the  postaddict's 
attempts  to  become  productive  and  responsible. 

Other  factois  in  relapse  may  relate  to  the  physical  changes  wrought  by 
prolonged  use  of  a  narcotic.  For  months  or  years  the  addict  sm  rendered 
himself  many  times  a  day  to  the  pharmacologic  thralldom  of  his  drug.  It  is 
possible,  indeed  it  now  seems  likely,  that  he  does  not  regain  his  foimer 
physical  status  after  the  7-10  days  required  to  detoxify  him  and  lite  few 
months  in  a  duig-free  environment  needed  to  free  him  of  all  obvious  signs 
of  addiction.  Dr.  William  R.  Martin,  director  of  the  Addiction  Research 
Center,  is  now  in  the  midst  of  a  detailed  study  of  the  effects  and  after- 
effects of  chronic  narcotic  addiction  in  man.  The  classic  study  done  in  (he 
mid-1 930's  by  C.  K.  Himmelsbach,  the  fiist  director  of  the  Cenlcr, 
showed  that  as  long  as  6  months  after  withdrawal  the  postaddict  is  hyper- 
reactive  to  some  physical  stimuli. 

Then  there  is  the  habit  itself,  a  pattern  of  behavior  that  becomes  a  way 
of  life  for  the  addict,  pivoting  on  the  compulsive  acquistion  and  use  oi'  the 
duig.  The  narcotic  habit  is  embedded  in  fundamentals  of  character,  social 
backgiound,  and  neurophysiological  functioning,  and  breaking  it  may 
prove  insupeiable  for  many  postaddicts.  To  account  in  part  for  this  aspect 
of  relapse  after  cure,  a  two-factor  learning  theory  has  been  proposed  by 
Dr.  Abraham  Wikler.  Dr.  Wikler  is  professoi  of  psychiatry  at  the  Univei- 
sity  of  Kentucky  College  of  Medicine  and  consultant  to  the  Addiction 
Research  Center.  For  many  ycais  he  served  as  chief  of  the  Center's  section 
on  expeiimental  neuropsychiatiy. 

Postaddicts  have  desciibed  experiencing  physical  symptoms  very  like 
those  of  acute  drug  abstinence  when  they  return  to  their  home  environ- 
ments many  weeks  or  months  after  withdrawal.  These  reports  suggested  to 
Dr.  Wiklei  that  physical  dependence  on  the  narcotic  drug  might  become 
conditioned  dining  the  course  of  addiction  to  certain  situations  in  I  he 
addict's  enviionment  This  conditioning  would  follow  the  Pavloviim 
model,  in  which  a  dog  came  to  salivate  at  the  sound  of  a  bell  that  had  rung 
many  times  before  in  temporal  contiguity  with  the  presentation  of  meal 
powder. 

The  investigator  reasoned  that  Pavlovian  conditioning  could  occur  when 
the  addict  failed  to  get  his  drug  in  time  to  avert  the  emergence  of"  with- 
drawal symptoms,  which  might  begin  within  3  or  4  hours  after  the  last 
dose.  A  conditioned  response  could  be  engendered  through  repealed 
temporal  associations  between  such  sporadic,  accidental  abstinences  iiml 
specific  environments,  for  example,  a  street  corner,  room,  or  bar,  Thus, 

O1'° '""'  '"  had  been  withdrawn  from  the  drug  for  some  time,  when 

HA  cellular  logic  for  abstinence  symptoms,  Uic 
•»f  past  abstinence  might  trigger  in  him 
'  mimicked  real  abstinence. 
•  hypothesizes  in  relapse  is  antilogous 
ierant  conditioning.  In  this  model  uu 
sously  (he  "operates"  on  his  cnviron- 
led  by  him  as  positive  or  negative.  If 


the  consequence  is  positive  and  rewarding,  the  theory  holds,  it  will 
reinforce  the  act  that  proceeded  it.  That  is,  the  act  will  be  repeated  if  it 
culminates  in  reward  often  enough.  The  addict's  frequent  intermittent 
bouts  of  abstinence  are  totally  and  dramatically  relieved  when  he  does 
succeed  in  acquiring  and  using  the  drug.  Thus,  his  drug-acquisitory 
behavior  is  reinforced  again  and  again  by  the  reward  of  relief  from 
abstinence  symptoms. 

Both  kinds  of  conditioned  learning  may  influence  behavior  in  the  same 
individual.  Dr.  Wilder  further  postulates  that  the  permanence  of  this  leatn- 
ing'is  directly  related  to  the  amount  of  effort  put  into  the  acquisition  of 
drugs.  Here  again  the  investigatoi  took  his  lead  from  the  reports  of  former 
addicts,  who  often  brag  among  themselves  about  how  hard  they  "hustled" 
(almost  always  ciiminally)  and  how  good  they  were  at  getting  their  supply 
when  they  were  "on  the  street."  Dr.  Wikler  suggests  that  the  status 
accorded  to  a  successful  hustler  by  addict  society  is  a  potent  secondary 
reinforcer  of  drug-acquisitory  behavior.  Scheduling  of  reinforcement  and 
stimulus  generalization  are  other  concepts  of  conditioned  learning  theory 
that,  Dr.  Wikler  believes,  also  are  peitinent  to  the  role  of  learning  in 
relapse  after  cure. 

Addiction  and  Relapse  in  Rats 

Experiments  to  test  the  Pavlovian  and  operant  learning  hypotheses  were 
done  with  rats.  The  studies  involved  these  basic  design  elements;  Rats  that 
weie  physically  dependent  on  morphine,  a  suitable  reinforcing  agent, 
methods  for  conditioning  the  animals,  and  testing  procedures  to  deter- 
mine whether  conditioning  had  occurred. 

Rats  were  made  physically  dependent  on  morphine  by  injecting  them 
with  the  drug  once  a  day  at  about  8  a.m.  Doses  were  gradually  increased 
over  a  period  of  6  weeks  until  the  desired  high-dose  level  was  reached.  The 
animals  then  were  maintained  on  this  dosage.  They  wore  addicted.  Control 
rats  weie  treated  similarly,  except  that  their  morning  injection  was  an 
innocuous  saline  solution.  By  evening  the  addict  rats'  doses  were  wearing 
off  and  they  were  exhibiting  signs  of  abstinence  including  "wet-dog 
shakes."  The  phrase  aptly  conveys  the  rat's  movement:  A  shaking  and 
twitching  of  trunk  and  limbs  that  resemble  a  wet  dog  shaking  water  from 
itself.  Normal  rats  briefly  Behave  this  way  after  handling.  Earlier  work  by 
Dr.  Wikler  and  his  colleagues  had  shown  that  elevated  wet-dog  frequencies 
were  reliable  signs  of  early  abstinence  in  the  chronically  addicted  rat, 
roughly  paralleling  such  other  abstinence  symptoms  as  loss  of  weight  and 
low  body  temperature,  increased  activity  and  elimination.  Wet-dog  shakes 
may  be  the  murine  equivalent  of  a  human  abstinence  symptom  called,  in 
another  graphic  phrase,  "cold  turkey."  Addicts  use  it  to  refer  to  wave 
after  chilling  wave  of  gooseflesh  (medically  termed  piloerection)  and  to 
describe  the  whole  range  of  symptoms  that  occurs  on  abrupt  termination 
of  narcotic  use. 

443 


The  reinforcing  agent  had  to  be  an  opiate  or  any  other  chemical 
compound  that  exerted  opiate-like  effects.  Morphine  itself  was  the  logical 
choice.  It  proved  to  be  unsatisfactory,  however,  presumably  because  the 
quantities  required  for  reinforcing  purposes  and  the  methods  of  delivery 
to  the  rats  were  aversive  to  the  animals.  Then  the  investigators  learned  of  a 
synthetic  drug  called  etonitazene,  which  was  just  being  described, 
Etonitazene  produced  morphine-like  effects  in  animals.  It  was  shown  to  be 
1,000  times  as  potent  as  moiphine  as  an  analgesic  in  the  rat.  The  re- 
searchers undertook  a  series  of  experiments  with  this  interesting  com- 
pound to  deteimine  if  it  could  serve  as  the  reinforcing  agent  in  condition- 
ing studies. 

Their  pieliminary  observations  were  encouraging.  First,  they  found  that 
rats  would  drink  a  dilute  water  solution  of  etonitazene  without  being 
forced  to,  that  is,  without  prior  water  deprivation.  Water  deprivation  had 
been  a  complicating  but  necessary  procedure  when  morphine  was  tried  as 
the  reinforcing  agent.  If  etonitazene  were  used,  water  deprivation  could  be 
eliminated.  Second,  drug-treated  animals  that  were  abstinent  24-48  hours 
drank  much  more  water  if  it  contained  etonitazene  than  if  it  were  plain, 
and  abstinent  rats  consuming  etonitazene  solution  showed  lower  wet-clog 
frequencies  than  those  drinking  water.  It  looked  as  if  the  grealci 
consumption  of  etonitazene  solution  compared  to  water  might  have 
resulted  from  the  drug's  power  to  suppress  abstinence  discomfort.  Further 
study  demonstrated  that  this  was  indeed  so. 

In  normal,  control  rats,  fluid  consumption  was  not  affected  when  a 
solution  containing  5  or  10  micrograms  of  etonitazene  per  milliliter  of 
water  was  substituted  for  plain  water.  Nor  was  fluid  consumption  altered 
when  the  substitution  was  made  for  animals  in  a  state  of  morphine 
intoxication,  a  period  lasting  about  7  hours  from  the  time  of  the  single 
daily  drug  injection.  However,  experimental  animals  showing  morphine- 
abstinence  symptoms,  which  began  about  the  13th  hour  after  injection 
and  continued  until  the  next  morning  injection,  drank  much  greater 
volumes  when  their  fluid  was  etonitazene  solution  than  when  it  was  water. 
Moreover,  the  usual  primary  abstinence  symptoms  did  not  appear  when 
experimental  animals  in  a  condition  of  emerging  abstinence  were  allowed 
to  drink  drug  solution.  The  investigators  reasoned  that  etonitazene 
solution  was  consumed  in  greater  quantities  than  water  by  morphine- 
abstinent  rats  because  for  them-and  only  for  them-it  had  reinforcing 
properties,  specifically,  relief  from  abstinence  discomforts.  In  sum, 
etonitazene's  various  attributes  made  it  an  excellent  choice  for  the  role  of 
reinforcing  agent. 

The  procedures  devised  to  condition  animals  were  carried  out  in  in- 
dividual linear  mazes.  Mazes  were  constructed  so  that  an  animal's  access  to 
one  or  the  other  end  could  be  blocked.  On  certain  days  of  the  week,  from 
the  time  of  morning  injection  until  2  p.m.,  all  rats  had  free  run  of  their 
mazes  and  could  drink  plain  water  available  at  either  end.  From  2  p.m. 
until  the  next  morning  they  were  allowed  access  to  only  one  end  where, 
again,  they  found  water  to  drink  at  will.  The  evening  routine  was  altered 
on  other  days  of  the  week:  From  about  8  p.m.  until  the  next  morning  all 

444 


rats  were  confined  to  the  opposite  maze  end.  The  fluid  available  for 
drinking  here  was  different  for  each  of  four  subgroups.  In  every  maze,  this 
end  also  was  cued  to  help  the  animal  discriminate  it  from  the  other  end. 
Anise  flavoi  tagged  the  drinking  fluid  in  the  first  study;  in  a  replicate 
study,  visual  and  tactile  cues  were  used  instead. 

•  Drinking  water  available  to  one  group  of  morphine-addicted  rats 
contained   10  micrograms  of  etonitazene  in  every  milliliter  of  water,  a 
concentration  sufficient  to  relieve  abstinence. 

•  A  second  group  of  morphine-addicted  rats  had  no  etonitazene  in 
their  drinking  water.  Only  the  discriminative   cues  marked  this  end  as 
different  from  the  other  end  for  them. 

•  One  control  group  had  available  for  drinking  a  solution  containing 
half  the  amount  of  etonitazene  placed  in  the  first  addict  group's  water. 
The  smaller  potency   reduced  the  chances  of  these  animals  becoming 
tolerant  to,  or  dependent  on  the  drug. 

«  A  second  control  group  was  placed  in  the  same  situation  as  that  of 
the  second  addict  group. 

Two  basic  strategies  were  behind  these  rather  complicated  procedures. 
First,  both  groups  of  addict  rats  suffered  withdrawal  symptoms  nightly  in 
the  mazes;  they  were  conditioned  to  the  mazes  as  an  environment  of 
abstinence.  Second,  one  addict  group  was  conditioned  on  alternate  nights 
to  associate  relief  from  abstinence  with  the  behavior  of  drinking  water 
(containing  etonitazene),  tagged  with  certain  cues,  while  the  second  addict 
group  was  never  allowed  to  relieve  abstinence  and  therefore  made  no 
association  between  relief  and  the  behavior  of  drinking  water  (not 
containing  etonitazene),  tagged  with  the  same  cues.  The  first  group  was 
trained  in  the  operant  conditioning  model  and  the  second  was  not. 

Conditioning  procedures  were  carried  out  for  6  consecutive  weeks. 
Animals  remained  in  mazes  the  following  week  but  were  not  allowed 
access  to  the  cued  end-compartments.  This  was  a  precautionary  step  to 
equalize  the  degrees  of  addiction  of  the  two  experimental  subgroups;  in 
case  the  one  that  had  consumed  etonitazene  solution  every  other  night 
during  conditioning  had  acquired  a  stronger  drug-dependence  than  the 
subgroup  that  received  only  the  morphine  injections. 

Then  all  injections  were  halted  and  animals  were  returned  to  their  home 
cages.  All  drug-treated  rats  underwent  abrupt  withdrawal  and  showed  the 
typical  acute  abstinence  symptoms.  Beginning  about  a  week  after 
injections  were  stopped  and  continuing  at  intervals  up  to  several  months 
afterward,  the  animals  were  observed  in  a  battery  of  procedures  and 
relapse  tests  to  determine  the  effect  of  training  in  relation  to  previous 
treatment. 

Dr.  Wikler  and  his  coworker,  Frank  T.  Pescor,  a  biologist,  conducted 
two  complete  conditioning  studies,  retracing  each  step  of  treatment,  train- 
ing, and  testing.  The  second  study  differed  somewhat  in  details  from  the 
first,  which  is  the  one  described  above,  but  the  principles  of  each  were 
identical.  The  studies  yielded  comparable  results. 

The  investigators  found  that  wet-dog  frequencies  of  postaddict  rats 
were  higher  in  the  mazes  than  in  the  home  cages  long  after  ail  other  signs 

445 


of  primary  abstinence  had  subsided  and  postaddict  wet-dog  frequencies  in 
home  cages  had  returned  to  the  level  shown  by  controls.  In  some  relapse 
tests,  wet-dog  fiequencies  of  control  rats  changed  in  the  same  direction  as 
those  of  postaddict  rats,  but  the  amount  of  change  was  smaller  for 
controls.  Thus,  the  experiments  demonstrated  the  Pavlovian  condition- 
ability  of  this  abstinence  phenomenon  in  the  rat.  Considered  along  witii 
the  suggestive  clinical  reports  of  former  addicts,  the  experimental  evidence 
supports  the  hypothesis  that  abstinence  phenomena  may  be  similarly 
conditioned  in  man. 

Evidence  concerning  the  role  of  reinforced  ope  rant  behavior  in  relapse 
could  not  be  obtained.  During  the  test  period  of  the  study,  rats  were 
exposed  to  both  forced  drinking  of  dilute  etonitazene  water  and  free- 
choice  drinking  of  either  plain  or  doped  water.  According  to  the 
hypothesis,  those  postaddict  rats  which  had  learned  during  training  thnl 
the  doped  water  alleviated  their  discomfort  (abstinence  symptoms)  would 
choose  to  drink  more  doped  water  after  cure  than  would  postaddict  rats 
which  had  not  been  so  trained.  The  investigators  found  that,  although 
both  postaddict  groups  drank  significantly  more  doped  water  than  did 
control  rats,  there  was  no  difference  between  the  two  postaddict  groups  in 
the  amount  of  doped  water  consumed  by  free  choice  after  withdrawn!. 
Both  relapsed  after  cure,  but  the  investigators  could  not  find  any  dif- 
ference between  them  in  the  nature  of  their  relapse.  Dr.  Wikler  believes 
that  a  test  with  greater  discriminative  power  than  free-choice  drinking  will 
have  to  be  devised  to  expose  differences  in  relapse  liability  that  might 
exist  between  the  two  groups,  and  thereby  to  isolate  the  role  of  reinforced 
operant  behavior. 

The   experiment   did   demonstrate   that,   regardless  of  the  roles  of 
Pavlovian  and  operant  conditioning,  previous  addiction  itself  disposed  rats 
to  consume  etonitazene  solution  long  after  primary  withdrawal  symptoms 
had  subsided.  Dr.  Wikler  suggests  that  postaddict  rats'  greater  free-choice 
consumption  of  doped  water,  compared  with  the  amount  never-addicted 
rats  chose  to  drink,  may  have  been  due  to  prolonged  aftereffects  of  ad- 
diction. The  blatant,  well-recognized  symptoms  of  primary  abstinence  in 
the  chronically  addicted  rat  run  their  course  by  the  third  day  after  abrupt 
withdrawal,  but  earlier  studies  by  these  investigators  had  demonstrated 
so-called  secondary  abstinence  phenomena  in  the  chronically  addicted  nit, 
persisting  4-6  months  after  withdrawal.  Moreover,  they  had  found  that 
even  6  months  after  withdrawal  there  were  minor  differences  between 
postaddict  and  control  rats.  In  the  conditioning  study,  the  persistence  of 
elevated  wet-dog  frequencies  long  after  withdrawal  may  be  regarded  as  i\ 
manifestation  of  secondary  abstinence.  The  evidence  suggests  that  a  long- 
-g,  subtle  homeostatic  imbalance  in  postaddict  rats  is  an  important 
i  generating  relapse  after  cure.  Does  protracted  abstinence  occur  in 
/hen  Dr.  Martin  and  his  colleagues  complete  the  work  mentioned 
we  shall  be  able  to  answer  this  question  with  considerably  more 
ice  than  is  now  possible. 


Procedures  To  Break  the  Habit 

The  etiology  of  relapse  after  cure  is  certain  to  involve  interactions  of 
both  physiological  factois,  including  conditioned  learning  to  a  greater  or 
lesser  extent.  Every  promising  lead  must  be  followed;  until  we  understand 
why  postaddicts  relapse  after  cure  we  cannot  be  sure  that  the  prevailing 
methods  of  treating  narcotic  addiction  are  truly  curative.  If  postaddict 
patients  are  found  to  experience  long-enduring  residual  aftereffects,  for 
instance,  the  exact  duration  and  physiological  nature  of  the  effects  will 
need  to  be  determined  and  methods  of  specific  treatment  devised.  To  the 
extent  that  conditioning  factors  are  shown  to  contribute  to  relapse  after 
cure,  extinction  procedures  would  become  part  of  the  treatment  legimen. 
Dr.  Wikler  has  speculated  about  the  general  outlines  of  such  a  piogram.  He 
points  out  that  extinction  of  conditioned  abstinence  phenomena  and  of 
reinforced  drug-acquisitory  behavior  require  different  procedures  and 
should  be  done  separately,  beginning  with  the  latter. 

Two  new  drugs  would  be  needed.  One  would  be  used  with  incoming 
addicted  patients  during  the  detoxification  phase  of  treatment.  Its 
qualities  would  be  such  that  it  could  be  substituted  for  the  opiate  patients 
had  been  using.  On  withdiawal,  it  would  not  cause  the  emergence  of 
characteristic  heroin  or  morphine  withdrawal  phenomena.  Then  the  new 
drug  would  be  abruptly  withdrawn,  and  the  result  would  be  a  piolonged 
but  not  severe  abstinence  syndrome.  At  this  point  the  second  new  chug 
would  be  used.  This  drug  should  possess  some  rewarding  properties  so  that 
patients  would  work  for  it,  but  it  should  not  be  effective  in  relieving 
abstinence.  In  this  manner,  the  usual  reward  associated  with  drug- 
acquisitory  behavior  would  not  be  forthcoming,  and  the  old  conditioned 
stimulus-response  pattern  could  be  steadily  weakened. 

Dr.  Wiklei  proposes  that  rewards  to  condition  socially  acceptable 
operant  behavior  be  provided  in  separate  training.  Postaddicts  might  earn 
money  for  useful  work,  which  they  would  be  allowed  to  spend  for  things 
they  want  other  than  drugs. 

Extinction  of  conditioned  physical  dependence  would  begin  after  this 
hospital  program.  It  would  be  carried  out  in  the  postaddict's  home  en- 
vironment. Surveillance,  nonnarcotic  drug  therapy,  and  psychotherapy 
would  be  used  to  bolster  the  postaddict  and  forestall  relapse  should 
conditioned  abstinence  occur.  In  this  way,  the  classical  (Pavlovian)  condi- 
tioning factor  would  gradually  be  extinguished. 

Recent  studies  made  at  the  Addiction  Research  Center  have  provided 
another  possible  means  of  extinguishing  conditioned  abstinence  and  drug- 
seeking  behavior.  A  new  synthetic  substance— cyclazocine-seems  to  have 
especially  desirable  characteristics.  Dr.  Wikler  thinks  that  maintenance  of 
postaddict  patients  on  cyclazocine,  as  proposed  by  the  research  group 
headed  by  Dr.  Martin,1  would  lead  to  expeiimental  extinction  of  the 


'Martin,  W.  R.,  Gorodetzky,  C.  W.,  and  McClane,  T.  K.  Treatment  of  Narcotic 
Addicts  With  Cyclazocine,  Clin.  Pharmacol.,  Therap,,  1966,  7,455-465. 

447 


conditioned    abstinence    symptoms    and    drug-acquisitive    behavior   he 
postulates  are  factors  in  relapse. 

Cyclazodne  is  a  narcotic  antagonist,  a  compound  that  blocks  many  of 
an  opiate's  effects.  Like  several  of  the  narcotic  antagonists  known, 
cyclazocine  has  analgesic  properties;  indeed,  it  possesses  many  times  the 
analgesic  potency  of  morphine.  (It  has  been  found  to  produce  hallucina- 
tions in  man  frequently,  however,  and  this  disturbing  side  effect  makes  it 
unacceptable  as  an  analgesic  for  general  clinical  use.)  Another  significant 
property  of  many  of  these  agents  is  that  chronic  usage  does  not  produce 
tolerance  to  their  opiate  counteractions.  This  is  true  of  cyclazocine. 

The  Martin  group  found  that  subjects  (abstinent  postaddict  patients) 
who  had  been  chronically  intoxicated  and  then  withdrawn  from 
cyclazocine  experienced  only  mild  abstinence  symptoms  and  did  not  crave 
more  of  the  substance.  Further  work  demonstrated  that,  when  subjects 
were  taking  cyclazocine  chronically,  even  very  large  doses  of  morphine 
failed  to  produce  such  customary  effects  as  severe  respiratory  depression 
and  euphoria.  Under  these  circumstances  of  cyclazocine  premedication, 
the  patients  were  also  protected  fiom  developing  physical  dependence 
when  morphine  was  chronically  administered. 

The  researchers  outlined  how  cyclazocine  might  be  employed  wi!h 
abstinent  narcotic  addicts  trying  to  stay  free  of  opiates.  The  physically 
dependent  addict  first  must  be  withdrawn  from  his  drug  because  cycln- 
zocine  precipitates  a  violent  abstinence  syndrome  in  the  morphine- 
dependent  patient.  Then  cyclazocine  may  be  given  orally,  twice  a  day,  in 
small  doses  that  are  gradually  increased  to  the  desired  level,  The 
postaddict  may  be  maintained  at  this  level  until  he  is  judged  to  luivc 
gained  all  he  can  from  the  treatment;  then  the  agent  may  be  gradually 
withdrawn.  The  investigators  advised  that  withdrawal  from  narcotics  mul 
stabilization  on  cyclazocine  be  done  in  a  controlled  hospital  setting.  Treat- 
ment may  continue  in  an  ambulatory  setting  once  the  patient  has  reached 
the  stabilization  dose  level.  Other  types  of  care,  provided  by  psycho- 
therapists and  social  workers,  may  then  help  him  build  a  new  life  for 
himself. 

While  the  postaddict  patient  is  maintained  on  cyclazocine,  he  would  be 

protected  from  the  euphoria  of  even  large  doses  of  morphine  or  heroin 

taken,    perhaps,    while    on    a    spree-and    from    developing    physical 

dependence  on  opiates  again.  Cyclazocine  maintenance  thus  would  avert 

two  of  the  pharmacologic  actions  of  opiates  believed  to  be  imortanl  in 

narcotic  addiction.  "There  may  be  other  benefits,"  the  Martin  group 

Dr.  Wikler's  work,  they  concluded;  "It  is  possible  Unit 

™«+  to  r^dict  themselves  while  receiving  a  narcotic 

there  may  be  extinction  of  conditioned 

iking  behavior."2  Clinical  trials  of  cycla- 

addicts  are  currently  underway  in  New 


Intramural-  NIMH 

Date  of  Interview:  August,  1966 

References' 

Martin,  W.  R.,  Wilder,  A.,  Eades,  C.  G.,  and  Pescor,  F.  T  Tolerance  to  and  physical 
dependence  on  morphine  in  rats.  Psycho pharmacologia,  1963,  4,  247-260. 

Martin,  W.  R.,  Gorodetzky,  C.  W.,  and  McClane,  T  K.  Treatment  of  narcotic  addicts 
with  cyclazocine.  CUn.  Pharmacol.  Therap.,  1966,  7,  455-465. 

Wikler,  A.  Opiates  and  opiate  antagonists.  Public  Health  Monograph  No.  52  U.S 
Department  of  Health,  Education,  and  Welfare.  Washington,  D.C.,  !95S. 

Wikler,  A.  Conditioning  factors  in  opiate  addiction  and  relapse.  In  Kassenbaum,  G.  G. 
and  Wilner,  D.  M.  (eds.),  Narcotics.  New  York.  McGraw-Hill,  1965. 

Wilder,  A.,  and  Pescor,  F.  T.  Factois  disposing  to  "relapse"  in  rats  previously  addicted 
to  morphine  (abs.).  The  Pharmacologist*  1965,  7,  171. 

Wikler,  A.,  Martin,  W.  R.,  Pescor,  F.  T.,  and  Eades,  C.  G.  Factors  regulating  oral 
consumption  of  an  opioid  (etonitazene)  by  morphine-addicted  rats.  Psycho- 
pharmacologta,  1963,5,55-76. 

Wikler,  A.,  and  Pescor,  F.  T.  Classical  conditioning  of  a  morphine  abstinence 
phenomenon,  reinforcement  of  opiod-drinking  behavior  and  "relapse"  in  morphine- 
addicted  rais.Psychopharmacologia,  1967,  10,  3,255-284. 


449 


Investigators 

Constantine  J.  Falliers,  M.D. 

Kenneth  Purcell,  Ph.  D. 

William  W.  Hahn,Ph.  D, 

Children's  Asthma  Research  Institute  and  Hospital 

Denver,  Colo. 

Prepared  by: 

Gay  Luce 

The  Children's  Asthma  Institute  and  Hospital  is  widely  known  for  Us 
multidisciplinary  research  on  asthma,  and  for  outstanding  success  in  treat- 
ing severely  ill  children.  Partly  aided  by  NIMH,  investigators  there  arc 
studying  the  manner  in  which  emotions  and  physiology  interact  to  induce 
asthmatic  episodes.  Dr.  William  W.  Hahn  has  found  that  asthmatic 
children  tend  to  withdraw  from  criticism  and  to  respond  to  stress  with 
distinctive  respiratory  and  pulse  changes,  Currently,  he  is  monitoring  the 
sleep  of  asthmatic  children  in  order  to  determine  what  physiological 
and/or  psychological  states  in  sleep  are  correlated  with  night  attacks.  Dr. 
Kenneth  Purcell  has  devised  a  system  of  FM  monitoring  which  will  enable 
the  staff  to  review  a  child's  social  reactions  in  an  effort  to  sec  the 
emotional  antecedents  of  acute  attacks.  Although  asthmatic  attacks  muy 
be  triggered  by  some  combination  of  emotional  and  allergic  circum- 
stances, a  new  approach  has  been  to  scan  for  biological  rhythms  that  niny 
hold  clues  to  susceptibility.  A  recent  study  by  Dr.  Falliers  indicates  that 
theie  is  a  24-hour  rhythm  in  the  peak  expiratory  flow  rate—a  measure  of 
lung  function— in  asthmatic  children.  Furthermore,  the  timing  of  hormone 
therapy  alters  the  timing  of  this  peak. 

Innovations  in  long-distance  monitoring  by  FM  radio,  and  perhaps  later 
by  biotelemetry,  are  opening  up  new  possibilities  in  understanding  how 
emotions,  external  conditions,  and  perhaps  body  rhythms  conspire  to 
produce  the  crippling  symptoms  of  asthma  and  how  timing  may  become 
an  effective  element  in  future  treatment  of  the  illness. 


Background 


pling  and  dreadful  illnesses,  most  people 

This  is  odd,  because  asthma  or  related 

out  of  every  10  Americans.  Attacks  arc 

st  most  people  do  not  consider  asthma  a 

ailment.  In  point  of  fact,  it  causes  about 


9,000  deaths  a  year,  deaths  that  are  particularly  horrible  since  they  arise 
from  strangulation.  About  3  million  American  children  suffer  from 
asthma  at  one  time  or  another;  300,000  have  chronic  asthma,  and  many 
are  so  severely  ill  that  they  do  not  improve  under  conventional  treatment. 
They  miss  from  1  to  3  months  of  school  each  year,  cannot  join  in  the 
normal  physical  activities  of  their  peers,  and  are  retarded  in  physical 
growth.  Psychologically,  these  children  and  their  families  must  learn  to 
cope  with  a  frightful  life-and- death  emergency  that  can  suddenly  strike; 
A  child  in  an  acute  attack  may  gasp  for  breath  and  in  the  failuic  of  his 
respiratoiy  system,  begin  to  turn  blue-sometimes  requiring  the  oxygen 
equipment  of  a  police  department  to  save  his  life. 

The  Children's  Asthma  Research  Institute  and  Hospital  treats  about 
150  of  these  youngsters  from  all  over  the  country.  The  medical  program 
began  in  1939,  with  the  Jewish  National  Home  for  Asthmatic  Children, 
and  as  the  need  for  multidisciplinary  reseat ch  became  evident,  CARIH  was 
created  in  1959,  and  combined  residential  and  hospital  facilities  with 
research.  The  campus  resembles  a  private  boarding  school,  strewn  with 
small  residence  quarters,  playing  fields,  and  recreational  buildings.  Incon- 
spicuous among  the  buildings  is  a  modern  hospital  and  a  separate  research 
building.  The  critically  ill  children  who  come  to  CARIH,  for  periods  of 
18-24  months,  live  with  their  peers  in  groups,  attend  school,  and  receive 
an  unusual  regimen  of  psychological  and  medical  treatment,  and  various 
studies.  When  they  leave,  about  80  percent  require  much  less  medication 
than  before  to  help  them  lead  normal  lives,  and  the  remainder  can  lead 
normal  lives  with  the  help  of  drugs  judiciously  employed. 

The  success  of  CARIH,  indeed,  points  up  the  very  mysterious  nature  of 
the  illness  itself. 

In  1966,  a  group  of  boys,  who  had  been  too  ill  to  walk  to  school  before 
they  came  to  CARIH,  began  to  play  football  against  teams  of  healthy 
children.  They  not  only  played:  they  competed  and  won.  Bediidden  chil- 
dren had  been  transformed  into  sports  competitors.  The  staff  were  the 
first  to  admit  that  this  transformation  was  uncanny,  and  it  underscored 
their  problem  in  understanding  the  ailment.  Were  the  boys  enabled  to  play 
football  by  treatments  that  caused  physiological  development?  Were  they 
liberated  by  psychological  therapy  and  the  emotional  change  of  being 
away  from  home?  Was  there  something  generally  desirable  about  the 
Denver  environment?  "If  we  knew  for  sure,"  remarked  one  doctor,  "we'd 
soon  have  asthma  licked." 


Psychological  Role  of  the  Family 

Inevitably,  when  a  child  arrives  at  the  institute  he  enters  a  totally 
changed  world.  He  may  leave  behind  a  city  like  New  York,  with  its  partic- 
ular climate  and  air  pollution.  There  are  changes  in  pollen  characteristics, 
foods,  and  factors  such  as  the  molds  that  may  grow  in  his  basement  at 
home,  or  household  pets,  or  neighborhood  pets.  The  child  is  removed 
from  many  of  the  usual  physical  attributes  of  his  environment  that  may 

451 


trigger  or  contribute  to  his  respiratory  troubles.  Moreover,  he  leaves  what- 
ever emotional  conflicts  are  engendered  by  his  family.  About  a  third  of 
the  children  who  have  come  to  CARIH  have  improved  so  fast,  with  so 
little  aid  from  medication,  that  they  were  at  first  called  "mircale  children/1 
Their  improvement  suggested  that  emotional  factors  may  have  been  sing- 
ularly important  in  the  acuteness  of  their  illness. 

Asthma  has  long  been  considered  a  psychosomatic  disease  by  many 
investigators.  One  old  and  prevailing  theory  held  that  asthmatic  attacks 
were  the  bursting  forth  of  "suppressed  crying,"  and  the  asthmatic  child 
was  supposed  to  have  been  unable  to  express  certain  emotions.  Dr. 
Kenneth  Purcell  surveyed  the  interview  responses  of  CARIH  children 
about  the  causes  of  their  attacks.  A  number  of  them  felt  that  crying 
precipitated  an  attack  rather  than  relieving  it. 

Others  thought  that  laughing  or  coughing  brought  on  an  attack.  Thus  it 
appeared  that  general  excitation  of  the  respiratory  system  was  singled  out, 
and  it  seemed  highly  likely  that  these  children  learned  to  inhibit  crying > 
coughing,  or  laughing— precisely  in  order  to  avoid  an  attack.  It  was  the 
respiratory  excitation,  not  the  emotional  expression,  that  they  seemed  to 
want  to  avoid.  Suppressed  crying  might  indeed  be  a  learned  response, 
Similarly,  excessive  mother-child  dependency  has  been  cited  as  a  cause  of 
asthma:  Does  the  mother's  undue  attention  elicit  the  asthma,  or  is  her 
overprotectiveness  inspired  by  the  plight  of  her  asthmatic  child?  In  survey- 
ing the  literature  and  evaluating  the  CARIH  patients,  Dr.  Purcell  has  been 
struck  with  the  fact  that  asthma  is  by  no  means  homogeneous.  Pollens, 
dust,  colds,  foods,  or  climate  changes  precipitated  attacks.  Many  children 
have  unexplained  attacks  in  sleep,  and  physical  exertion  or  weather 
changes  were  common  piecipitants.  Still,  there  did  seem  to  be  some  dis- 
tinction between  the  so-called  "miracle  children"  who  promptly  remitted 
almost  on  arrival  at  CARIH  and  needed  no  medication— the  intermediate 
group  that  could  respond  to  therapy  without  relying  on  adrenocorticoid 
hormones-and  the  group  that  absolutely  depended  for  survival  upon 
some  hormone  therapy.  Dr.  Purcell  postulated  that  the  drug-dependeni 
children  may  have  been  more  influenced  by  infection  and  allergy  than  the 
others.  Perhaps  the  rapid  remitters  were  children  in  whom  symptoms  had 
a  predominantly  emotional  basis.  On  responses  to  questionnaires,  parents 
of  these  children  seemed  to  indicate  that  they  had  more  punitive  and 
authoritarian  attitudes  than  did  the  parents  of  the  other  youngsters.  Was 
family  conflict  at  the  bottom  of  their  allergic  disease? 

One  youngster,  who  had  improved  remarkably  on  arrival  at  CARIH, 
suffered  a  terrible  attack,  suddenly,  6  months  later.  His  family  had  come 
for  their  first  visit.  However,  if  the  staff  had  been  tempted  to  conclude 
that  this  was  a  prime  example  of  emotional  triggering- they  discovered 
that  the  family  cat  had  been  brought  along,  and  the  child  was  highly 
allergic  to  the  beast,  Thus,  the  interrelationship  of  environmental  factors 
had  made  it  difficult  to  pin  down  the  specific  triggers.  An  Ingenious  plan, 
involving  2  years  of  work  and  considerable  patience,  has  been  devised  in 
order  to  study  the  role  of  the  family  in  asthmatic  illness.  It  was  designed 

452 


to  hold  environment  constant,  so  that  the  asthmatic  child  experienced 
only  one  change— the  absence  01  the  presence  of  his  family. 

Children  suffering  from  chronic  asthma  in  the  Denver  area  were 
selected  for  this  study.  These  children  are  outpatients  of  CARIH,  who 
have  come  to  the  institute  for  daily  tests.  After  several  weeks  of 
evaluation,  the  families  were  offered  a  2-week  paid  vacation  in  a  hotel 
away  from  home.  During  this  time,  the  child  would  be  cared  for  and 
studied  by  a  surrogate  mother  from  the  CARIH  staff.  This  staff  member 
would  live  at  home  with  the  child,  with  the  daily  visits  to  CARIH  con- 
tinuing throughout  this  period  and  after  the  family's  return  home. 

There  have  been  22  children  studied  so  far.  Some  have  been  selected  on 
the  conjecture  that  they  would  show  improvement  when  their  families 
departed,  while  others  were  not  suspected  of  so  many  emotional  factors 
underlying  their  illness. 

About  70  percent  of  the  youngsters  in  the  emotional  group  did  actually 
improve  when  their  families  were  away  and  showed  a  resumption  of 
respiratory  symptoms  when  they  returned.  Only  10  percent  of  the 
children  whose  ailment  was  judged  as  less  overtly  emotional  showed 
change  that  could  be  correlated  with  the  presence  or  absence  of  the 
family.  Thus,  it  seems  that  there  are  at  least  two  kinds  of  asthmatic 
children;  those  in  whom  family  emotional  factors  play  a  role,  and  those  in 
whom  these  factors  do  not  seem  important.  These  two  groups  of  children 
are  now  being  compared  on  tests  of  autonomic  responses,  allergic  history  > 
and  a  variety  of  other  measures.  It  is,  however,  too  early  to  remark  on  the 
characteristics  that  distinguish  the  two  groups. 


Behavioral  Antecedents  of  the  Attack 

If  there  arc  emotional  interactions  that  precipitate  asthmatic  attacks, 
no  clear  correlation  has  been  found  among  studies  of  individual  patients. 
Teams  of  allergists,  psychologists,  and  pediatricians  have  concentrated  on 
a  few  youngsters,  obtaining  information  from  the  children's  school- 
teachers, house-parents,  physician,  and  psychotherapist.  By  converging  on 
all  the  various  aspects  of  the  child's  life  following  an  asthma  attack,  they 
had  hoped  to  single  out  the  piecipitating  factors.  Invariably  there  were 
complications,  such  as  changes  in  weather  or  social  environment,  that 
made  it  impossible  to  construct  a  convincing  link  between  the  attack  and 
the  situation  that  preceded  it.  Moreover,  there  was  no  comprehensive 
observation  of  the  child's  social  interactions,  excepting  when  he  was  in  the 
presence  of  an  adult,  and  knew  it. 

Dr.  Purcell  and  his  associates  decided  that  a  more  natural  and  inclusive 
mode  of  observation  might  be  developed  by  having  the  children  wear  FM 
transmitters.  All  of  their  vocalizations,  as  well  as  breathing  sounds,  could 
then  be  recorded,  and  in  the  event  of  an  asthmatic  episode,  the  antecedent 
behavior  could  be  heard  on  tape  recordings.  This  kind  of  field  observation 
without  observers  is  possible  with  a  miniature  radio  transmitter,  small  as  a 
cigarette  package,  that  can  be  worn  on  an  ordinary  belt.  It  will  transmit  to 

453 


receives  in  a  200-foot  range,  all  verbalization,  whispers,  ami 
piratoiy  sounds  to  signal  the  audible  signs  of  asthma  lie  attacks.  If  a  cliifd, 
so  observed,  were  to  have  20  attacks,  it  would  be  possible  to  examine  the 
portions  of  the  recoiding  just  preceding,  and  to  compare  llwsc  wift 
samples  taken  on  other  days  at  the  same  time.  Later,  the  investijuiluii 
might  be  able  to  re-create  situations  that  led  to  attacks,  substiinluiliim  the 
con-elation  between  behavior  and  respiratory  trouble  in  the  tohonitoiy, 
One  question,  however,  demanded  an  answer  before  such  a  slmly  could  Iw 
planned.  Would  the  children  become  self-conscious  and  dislorl  their  lv- 
navior  if  they  knew  they  were  being  bugged? 

A  pilot  study  now  seems  to  have  answered  that  question.  Lust  your.  16 
boys  and  1  0  gnls  were  selected  from  the  adolescent  groups  as  bcinn  |urlie- 
ulaily  self-conscious  and  therefore  good  to  study  and  test.  Half  ol  Hie 
youngsters  were  given  live  transmitters;  the  other  half  were  ftivvti  dum- 
mies. hey  wore  these  packages  for  an  hour  each  evening  for  1  0  ihiys,  .nij 
each  child  knew  whether  or  not  he  was  really  bugged  ,  but  had  been  avkcd 
not  to  tell  anybody.  At  the  end  of  the  test,  the  houscparcnls  xveu-  nsked 
D  Chlldre?  Wore  live  transmitter*  and  which  wore  dummy 


D 

f  ™    lr  P™'mably>  if  the  bugged  children  w^  behaving  iinion-nlly 
we     inS^r  H     T1?'  ^  WOUld  fac  apparent  to  the  houacimwnis.  who 
end  Whad  been  asked  to  fil1  OL't  bchovOr  sc.il«  on 


e  been  asked  to  fil1  OL't  bchovOr  sc.il«  on 

test  children  dunng  different  stages  in  the  1  0  clays. 


n^  Hve  transmitte''s  8«ve  every  evidence  »r 

bvf.  fief|m°nito/ln8  a«d  of  having  become  oblivious  In 

*     °rf 


-.wwu.  itJui^uvci,   til    tnfi   pnn    nf    i-Im    f-.:,.i     ^i        .  * 

than  chance  hleue^nPwhM/^  tna1'  the  houseparenis  did  no  Dodcr 
radio  tiansmitters  Beh»v  i-ff "dren  W0re  dummies  and  which  wine  live 
been  very  striking 'indeed  ^,1-1?°^  lf  there  WCre  any'  couUl  not  ljin'° 
were  far  more  sensitive  tM  cmidren  s  own  descriptions  of  (heir  Jtvlin^s 


c  . 

lost  any  effect  on  their  interac?fons  ton8ua««. the  monllorlim  MIOII 


™..Ci«:aii  wation  Fnr  chp*?  inrvestiSators  have  contracted  with 
Astern.  Beidnninp  i»  ^leTOtopl^  «f  a  campus-wide  tck- 


Sleep 

Mysteriously,  out  of  an  apparently  untroubled  sleep,  the  asthmatic 
patient  sometimes  awakens  gasping  foi  air,  feeling  himself  on  the  verge  of 
suffocation.  In  some  children  the  frequency  of  nocturnal  attacks  is  5-10 
times  as  great  as  those  occurring  during  the  day.  The  night  of  sleep  has 
been  one  phase  of  the  daily  round  in  which  CARIH  children  have  been 
mostly  unobserved,  but  perhaps  this  span  of  quiet  can  reveal  how  an 
attack  develops  and  why  it  occurs  when  it  does.  Dr.  William  W.  Hahn  has 
established  a  sleep-monitoring  laboratoiy,  within  the  research  building, 
with  a  lecording  room  in  which  childien  now  sleep  through  many  con- 
secutive nights,  while  their  brain  waves,  pulse,  and  respiration  are  continu- 
ously recorded. 

Sleep  has  been  found  to  consist  of  several  stages,  each  characterized  by 
particular  brain  wave  patterns  and  physiological  changes.  A  person  passes 
down  and  up  through  these  different  levels  of  consciousness  in  a  relatively 
predictable  manner  throughout  the  night.  Four  or  five  times  in  the  course 
of  a  night  the  progression  is  repeated,  and  at  each  level  the  sleeper's 
muscle  tone,  his  arousability,  and  his  psychological  experiences  appear  to 
be  different.  At  approximately  90-minute  intervals  the  eyes  begin  moving 
rapidly,  the  pulse  changes,  respiration  grows  irregular,  and  if  awakened 
now,  most  people  remember  vivid  dreams.  This  rapid-eye-movement  stage 
of  sleep  has  been  associated  with  the  gastric  secretions  of  ulcer  patients, 
with  attacks  of  nocturnal  angina,  and  some  investigators  have  wondered 
whether  it  may  also  be  associated  with  asthma.  Dr.  Hahn  has  been  looking 
for  a  possible  link  between  the  asthmatic  attack  and  a  particular  stage  of 
sleep.  So  far,  five  children  have  been  studied  for  a  total  of  12  nights  and 
some  identifiable  patterns  are  beginning  to  appear. 

On  nights  when  a  child  in  the  laboratory  is  experiencing  fairly  constant 
wheezing  he  will  be  likely  to  spend  very  little  time  in  deeper  stages  of 
sleep  and  proportionately  more  time  in  the  lighter  stages.  It  isn't  possible 
to  tell,  at  this  point,  whether  the  child  is  initially  restless,  tense,  or 
uncomfortable— and  his  resphatory  distress  is  a  result  of  this  psychophysi- 
ological  tension.  Or  does  the  distress  of  mild  asthma  and  an  impending 
attack  elicit  various  psychological  and  physical  stresses  in  the  child?  Dur- 
ing the  night  it  appears  that  the  awareness  of  discomfort,  to  the  point  of 
waking  up  and  doing  something  about  it,  may  vary  with  the  stage  of  sleep. 
In  the  laboratory  a  child  with  very  mild  asthma  symptoms  has  awakened 
from  a  light  stage  of  dreaming  sleep  to  request  a  nebulizer  (spray  medica- 
tion), yet  the  same  child  has  continued  to  sleep  for  an  hour  or  more, 
wheezing  and  showing  signs  of  greater  respiratory  distress  during  the  other 
stages  of  sleep. 

Sleep  study  has  a  unique  advantage  for  tracking  the  development  of  an 
asthmatic  attack.  This  is  the  one  long  period  of  time  when  a  child  will  stay 
still  without  complaint. 

455 


Psychophysiological  Reactivity  in  Asthmatic  and  Normal  Children 

Two  years  ago  Dr.  Hahn  and  his  associates  began  to  find  a  suggestive 
difference  between  asthmatic  and  normal  children  in  their  reactions  to 
mental  arithmetic,  interference  while  they  were  solving  problems,  and  a 
situation  in  which  they  anticipated  a  mild  electric  shock.  The  asthmatic 
youngsters  showed  a  notably  higher  heart  rate.  Careful  checking  indicated 
that  this  was  not  caused  by  acclimatization  to  the  Denver  altitude,  that  it 
could  not  be  attributed  to  lack  of  adaptation,  nor  to  medication.  The 
unusual  incidence  of  tachycardia  among  asthmatic  youngsters  suggested  a 
possible  malfunction  within  the  autonomic  nervous  system.  The  invcsti* 
gator  speculated  that  this  might  be  a  premonitory  signal,  the  first  sign  of 
an  abnormal  response  which  might  develop  into  an  asthmatic  attack  were 
the  stress  continued. 

In  a  recent  study,  normal  and  asthmatic  children  aged  10—14  were 
compared  as  they  underwent  more  severe  stresses,  The  18  CARIH  young- 
sters were  very  much  improved,  had  shown  virtually  no  symptoms  since 
shortly  after  their  arrival  at  the  institute,  and  were  taking  no  regular 
medication.  Their  counterparts  were  21  healthy  youngsters  who  were 
Denver  residents  participating  in  a  summer  recreation  program.  At  the 
outset,  each  boy  was  asked  to  check  a  list  of  words  that  approximated  the 
way  he  felt  most  of  the  time:  a  quick  scanning  for  attitudes  and  emotions. 
Then  the  youngster  was  wired  for  physiological  recordings  of  heart  iatc, 
finger  and  face  temperature,  finger  pulse  volume,  skin  resistance,  ami 
respiration.  As  they  lay  in  the  recording  room  they  received  four  kinds  of 
stimuli-tones,  shocks,  and  two  sets  of  arithmetic  problems  geared  for 
their  age  level. 

The  problem  was  played  to  the  child  by  tape  recorder,  and  he  was 
asked  to  solve  it  mentally  and  announce  the  answer.  After  each  answer,  «n 
voice  would  make  some  remark  which,  unknown  to  the  child,  was  pre- 
recorded. Following  responses  to  the  first  set  of  problems,  the  rcrmirk; 
were  neutral  or  encouraging.  However,  during  the  second  batch  of  prob- 
lems the  child  heard  increasingly  severe  criticism.  "I  wonder  if  you'rt 
really  trying-most  children  your  age  don't  have  any  trouble  with  these 
problems." 

After  the  session  the  boy  was  questioned.  Did  he  realize  that  the  voice 
was  a  tape  recording?  Was  he  aware  that  ciritcism  was  part  of  the  oxpcri 
ment?  Did  he  realize  that  his  answers  actually  had  been  correct?  The 
questions  began  with  an  exploration  of  his  feelings,  his  reactions  to  UK 
criticism.  Once  the  child  learned  that  the  criticism  was  a  tape-recorclee 
dupe  he  was  usually  relieved  and  more  accurate  about  describing  his  feel 
ings.  In  general,  the  asthmatic  children  seemed  to  have  been  more  limit 
and  self-reproachful  when  they  were  criticized.  They  were  apt  to  say  thn 
they  had  gotten  angry  at  themselves,  that  they  felt  discouraged  aiu 
wondered  why  they  were  giving  wrong  answers,  The  normal  youngster: 
more  typically  believed  that  the  voice  had  to  be  kidding  or  putting  then 
on.  They  didn't  doubt  their  ability  to  compute  the  problems  and  tcnclcc 
to  be  angry  at  the  invisible  critic. 

456 


A  close  look  at  their  physiological  records  indicated  a  curious  differ- 
ence between  the  asthmatic  and  normal  boys.  The  asthmatics  showed  a 
generally  higher  heait  rate,  but  their  respiration  exhibited  a  curious  pat- 
tern as  the  stiess  mounted.  The  normal  children  showed  an  elevated  respi- 
ration rate,  but  the  asthmatics  reacted  by  breathing  faster  up  to  a  point 
and  then,  as  if  this  were  a  kind  of  ceiling,  or  as  if  they  exerted  control, 
their  lespiration  became  no  faster.  Slowed  expiration  is  one  of  the 
symptoms  of  an  asthmatic  attack.  It  is  interesting  that  the  physiological 
response  to  criticism  resembled,  in  this  manner,  the  initial  stages  of  the 
asthmatic  attack. 

The  psychological  after  re  act  ions  of  the  asthmatic  children,  as  judged  by 
the  checklist  of  adjectives  of  mood,  showed  a  preponderance  of  negative 
answers  when  once  again  they  were  asked  to  mark  the  words  that  de- 
scribed their  usual  state  of  mind.  Psychologically,  as  physiologically,  they 
gave  a  different  lesponse  to  the  stress  of  adverse  criticism  in  comparison 
with  nonasthmatic  peers, 

In  future  studies  the  investigator  will  attempt  to  discover  what  this 
difference  means.  One  important  possibility  is  that  the  emotional  reac- 
tions of  inward  anger  and  n  on  aggressive  self-reproach  are  stress  reactions 
functionally  related  to  the  asthma  attack,  and  not  merely  a  byproduct  of 
the  illness.  "Although  we  cannot  document  a  functional  or  causal  relation- 
ship, data  from  the  present  study  illustrate  that  a  complex  psychological 
stimulus  initiates  emotional  and  physiological  responses  in  the  asthmatic 
child  which  differ  from  those  aroused  in  normal  children  exposed  to  the 
same  conditions  or  stimulation." 


Rhythmicity  in  Physiological  Responses  and  Drug  Effects 

CARIH  children,  with  all  the  study,  monitoring,  and  therapy  they  re- 
ceive, might  seem  to  lead  extremely  abnormal  lives.  Yet  for  every  one  of 
these  children,  life  had  never  before  been  so  normal.  Medically  hopeless, 
always  in  jeopardy  of  strangling  attacks,  these  children  had  lived  a  more 
disabling  life  at  home.  There,  a  slight  wheezing  might  be  sufficient  to 
bring  tension  and  fear  into  the  whole  family.  It  is  clear  why  the  particular 
symptoms  of  this  disease  incur  strong  psychological  repercussions. 

At  home  a  child  might  have  to  be  rescued  by  the  police,  or  taken  to  the 
hospital  in  an  acute  attack,  stricken,  gasping,  and  turning  blue.  These 
episodes  sometimes  last  a  few  hours,  and  sometimes  linger  for  as  long  as  4 
days,  Special  equipment  is  needed,  for  the  victim's  chest  would  expand, 
trapping  air,  his  lungs  filling  with  thick  mucus.  At  the  CARIH  a  child  in 
such  trouble  would  immediately  receive  aid  from  a  pressure-breathing 
instrument,  from  chemical  sprays  to  open  his  bronchial  tubes  and  lungs, 
and  from  an  instrument  designed  to  remove  plugs  of  mucus  from  the 
lungs.  He  would  be  attended  by  calm  competent  nurses  and  doctors,  at 
once  soothing  him  and  also  studying  the  nature  of  his  attack.  To  a  child 
who  had  experienced  the  emergency  measures  that  all  of  the  children 
knew,  the  gamut  of  laboratory  research  tests  was  not  particulary  imposing. 

457 


Each  of  the  children  was  studied  along  many  dimensions  throughout  hi 
stay.  Within  the  laboratory,  the  children  were  exposed  to  changes  ii 
temperature,  humidity,  barometric  pressure,  and  psychological  provoc; 
tion-in  attempts  to  single  out  the  triggering  factor  in  their  allergy.  Th 
general  finding  was  never  a  single  factor,  but  a  combination  of  simultimi 
ous  changes  that  might  be  very  slight.  Each  child  regularly  takes  tests  c 
pulmonary  function  during  laboratory  exercise  and  controlled  climat 
changes.  Analyses  are  made  of  bacteriological  samples  from  the  respiratoi 
tracts.  Tests  of  pituitary-adrenal  function  and  immunological  factors  ai 
continued,  along  with  tests  of  particular  nose  spiays  used  to  dilate  tl 
bionchial  tubes,  and  studies  of  the  manner  in  which  these  dilators  nu 
affect  the  nervous  system  and  catecholamine  metabolism.  For  the  child 
CAREH,  life  as  a  continued  object  of  research  is  still  more  normal  thi 
was  the  disabled  life  of  a  youngster  always  on  the  precipice. 

In  their  efforts  to  uncover  the  origins  of  asthma,  and  hopefully  sor 
unique  factor  which  might  be  medically  countered,  the  CARIH  researc 
ers  have  taken  a  new  and  unusual  path.  They  have  been  studying  o 
dimension  that  is  usually  overlooked  in  medicine:  the  periodicity  of 
ternal  functions  and  of  physiological  responses.  An  almost  24-hou 
circadian— fluctuation  has  been  found  to  be  the  endogenous  charactci  is 
of  many  organs,  cells,  and  subcellular  activity  in  animals  and  man.  Abe 
every  24  hours,  body  temperature  rises  to  a  peak  and  falls  to  its  nac 
repeating  this  fluctuation  day  after  day  in  the  normal  person.  The  level 
certain  adrenal  hormones  in  the  blood,  or  excreted  in  urine,  also  show 
roughly  24-hour  rhythm.  At  almost  any  level,  from  large  body  syste 
down  to  cell  division,  rhythm  seems  to  be  an  important  property  of  i 
body's  organization.  The  circadian,  or  almost  daily  rhythm,  is  nearly 
prominent  among  certain  physiological  functions  as  is  the  mcnslr 
rhythm.  In  a  manner  remotely  analogous  to  the  winding  of  a  clock 
person's  environment,  his  regimen  of  sleep  and  waking,  of  meals  and  so 
activity,  seem  to  synchronize  his  multitudinous  internal  rhythms. 

Although  new  in  application  to  medicine,  the  relevance  of  circadiim  i 
other  biological  rhythms  has  been  amply  demonstrated  by  a  numbei 
scientists,  notably  by  more  than  a  decade  of  research  on  the  part  of 
Franz  Halberg  and  his  associates  at  the  University  of  Minnesota,  Not  o 
must  the  body  deliver  the  right  substances  to  the  right  places  in 
correct  amounts-but  it  must  do  it  at  the  right  time.  In  the  case  of 
victim  of  Cushing's  disease,  for  instance,  a  clue  in  diagnosis  is  the  rise 
fall  of  circulating  adrenal  steroids,  for  it  is  the  inverse  of  a  norma  I  rhy 
even  when  the  levels  remain  within  the  so-called  normal  range.  PI 
relationships  among  body  rhythms  can  also  provide  meaningful  chic 
pathology. 

Two  years  ago,  Dr.  Constantino  J.  Falliers  and  Dr.  Halberg  took  a 
step  in  evaluating  circadian  rhythms  in  asthmatic  and  normal  child 
They  compared  these  two  groups  of  children  on  certain  major  body  I 
tuations  as  they  occurred  around  the  clock. 

In  January  1966,  19  boys  at  CARIH-all  of  them  youngsters  who  s 
free  of  symptoms  and  who  required  no  medication-began  to  be  the  f< 

458 


of  special  and  continuing  interest.  During  the  daytime,  every  4  hours,  they 
were  briefly  interrupted  by  a  staff  member,  and  were  asked  to  bieathe 
into  a  tube  as  haid  and  rapidly  as  they  could.  This  flowmeter  measured 
the  number  of  liters  of  air  they  could  expel  from  their  lungs  each  minute. 
The  peak  rate  of  expiratory  flow  is  an  indicator  of  pulmonary  function. 
An  asthmatic  person,  whose  respiratory  passages  are  often  blocked,  may 
expire  too  little,  too  slowly.  During  stress  or  an  oncoming  attack,  expira- 
tion rate  diminishes  and  air  collects  in  the  lungs.  Thus  a  high  peak  expira- 
tory flow  rate  is  a  welcome  sign.  As  these  studies  were  to  demonstrate,  the 
peak  is  not  the  same  at  different  times  of  day,  even  within  an  individual 
who  is  noimal.  One  question  that  interested  the  CARIH  staff  was  whether 
normal  and  asthmatic  children  would  show  their  peaks  at  the  same  points 
on  the  daily  round. 

In  April,  and  again  in  May,  the  boys  underwent  more  than  their  daily 
tests.  Now,  around  the  clock  for  48  hours,  they  were  tested  on  several 
physiological  changes.  Seventeen  normal  boys  of  the  same  age,  orphans  in 
Denver  institutions,  were  brought  to  the  institute  for  scrutiny  and  com- 
parison. At  each  age  level,  the  boys  lived  the  same  schedule  of  mealtimes, 
bedtime,  and  waking.  For  a  little  more  than  2  days  they  were  housed  in  a 
rescaich  wing  of  the  hospital  on  campus.  There  they  lived  in  scaled  rooms 
under  the  constant  supervision  of  nurses.  They  could  not  even  go  to  the 
bathroom  unsupervised,  since  all  urine  was  being  collected  in  4-hour 
samples  for  analysis  of  17-hydroxycortiscosteroids. 

Beginning  at  midnight,  and  every  4  hours  thereafter,  the  boys  were 
briefly  interrupted  in  their  sleep  or  activities  for  a  measure  of  blood 
pressure,  pulse,  a  urine  sample,  and  rectal  temperature.  They  were  asked 
to  breathe  into  the  expiratoiy  flowmeter,  and  to  estimate  when  2 
minutes  had  passed  by  counting  to  120.  This  ubiquitous  little  test  indi- 
cates approximately  whether  a  peison  senses  the  passage  of  time  as  slow, 
fast,  or  indeed  as  the  stopwatch  reads.  In  a  relatively  predictable  fashion, 
all  of  these  physiological  variables  and  responses  cliange  around  the  24 
hours. 

It  is,  of  course,  far  easier  to  describe  in  writing  a  48-hour  study  of  3 
dozen  youngsters  than  to  confront  the  actual  doing.  When  measures  must 
be  taken  at  equally  spaced  intervals,  around  the  clock,  and  when  precision 
is  crucial,  staff  coordination  and  exertion  is  intense.  Many  staff  members 
volunteered  to  work  on  the  2-day  marathons,  around  the  clock;  an  ex- 
hausting procedure  for  all. 

Throughout  the  longitudinal  studies  as  well,  it  was  necessary  to  chase 
the  boys  down  promptly  every  4  hours,  whether  they  were  out  playing, 
roller  skating,  or  swimming.  A  staff  member  would  have  to  find  them  and 
make  them  blow  into  the  flowmeter. 

The  very  arduousness  of  such  a  study  prescribes  a  very  cautious  experi- 
mental design.  There  are  limits  to  the  endurance  of  even  a  mildly  sick 
child,  and  limits  to  the  energies  of  the  staff.  Thus,  it  is  necessary  to 
ascertain  beforehand  how  many  samples,  at  what  intervals,  are  needed  to 
determine  the  peak  of  a  circadian  rhythm,  and  by  what  analysis  the  fre- 
quency and  peak  phase  will  become  clear.  A  longitudinal  study  of  certain 

459 


functions  could  be  run  for  several  months  with  the  asthmatic  children  aj 
one  normal  child,  but  measures  could  only  be  taken  during  daytim 
Interruption  of  sleep  over  several  months  would  not  only  be  likely  to  alt 
the  rhythms  under  study,  but  also  to  exacerbate  the  children's  symptoi 
and  subject  the  staff  to  extreme  exhaustion.  Thus,  within  the  framewo 
of  an  unevenly  paced  longitudinal  study  containing  nightly  gaps,  she 
intensive  studies  were  conducted  around  the  clock.  By  suitable  analys 
performed  by  computer,  these  could  fill  in  some  of  the  gaps.  As  I 
Falliers  said  of  his  collaboration,  the  study  could  not  have  been  feasible 
a  small  institution  without  the  sophisticated  statistical  help  and  the  co 
puter  analysis  offered  by  the  Minnesota  laboratory. 

In  comparing  the  normal  and  asthmatic  children,  the  researchers  ch< 
to  look  for  the  highest  daily  values-trie  peak  expiratory  flow  rate,  th 
horns  in  which  adrenal  steroids  were  excreted  most,  the  point  of  high 
pulse  or  blood  pressure,  etc.  These  crests  were  then  plotted  on  a  24-h< 
compass,  and  it  was  easy  to  see  how  the  two  groups  differed  or  coincid 
The  normal  children,  for  instance,  were  close  to  one  another  in  the  dis 
bution  of  their  highest  body  temperatures,  which  occurred  during  af 
noon  and  early  evening.  The  asthmatic  children  were  even  more  hoi 
geneous,  showing  their  peaks  exclusively  in  the  afternoon.  Their  fas' 
pulse,  however,  was  more  distributed  over  the  day  than  was  the  peak  h< 
rate  of  the  normal  youngsters.  Again,  in  the  excretion  of  adrenal  stero 
the  healthy  boys  showed  their  peaks  between  8  a.m.  and  1  p.m.,  whi 
few  asthmatic  children  exhibited  peaks  very  early  in  the  morning  o 
early  afternoon.  Although  a  harmonic  analysis  of  the  data  did  indi< 
some  differences  between  the  groups,  in  the  crest  phase  of  their  phj/ 
logical  functions,  no  further  conclusions  could  be  drawn  from  sue 
preliminary  study,  Perhaps  the  asthmatic  children  as  a  group  showt 
little  more  variability  in  circadian  crests. 

On  the  other  hand,  the  circadian  rhythm  of  peak  lung  functioning  t 
seem  to  differ  considerably  among  asthmatic  and  normal  groups.  In  in 
sive  48-hour  studies,  and  in  longitudinal  studies,  it  has  become  appa 
that  there  are  circadian  fluctuations  in  the  peak  expiratory  flow  ] 
Healthy  children  showed  crests  between  1  and  4  p.m.,  while  the  ave 
peak  of  the  asthmatic  child  occurred  around  2  p.m.  Here,  the  hea 
children  were  more  heterogeneous  and  exhibited  more  individual  v 
tion. 

Further  data,  still  in  analysis,  were  obtained  from  May  until  the  en 
September  1966.  Every  other  week,  the  17  boys  and  one  normal  gi 
this  study  underwent  48  hours  of  round-the-clock  study. 


Circadian  Rhythms  in  Drug  Effects 

Adrenal  hormones  play  an  important  role  in  the  therapy  of  asf 
sometimes  in  severe  cases  cortisone  and  its  analogs  are  the  only  me 
tions  that  make  life  possible.  Judged  by  available  techniques,  asthi 
youngsters  do  not  seem  to  suffer  from  adrenal  insufficiency,  an< 

460 


steroids  they  take— not  for  correction  of  a  deficiency  but  for  their  potent 
anti-inflammatory  action— have  many  side  effects.  One  of  these  appears 
to  be  a  dampening  effect  upon  growth.  These  youngsters  are  usually  be- 
hind their  peers  in  growth  and  maturation,  although  tests  at  CARIH  indi- 
cate that  they  have  the  normal  levels  of  growth  hormone.  Some  of  them 
have  been  given  anabolic  hormones  to  stimulate  growth.  Still,  the  side 
effects  of  their  lifegiving  hormone  treatments  persist  at  high  dosages.  Al- 
though the  corticosteroid  drugs  have  been  available  for  more  than  1 5 
years,  no  definitive  recommendation  can  yet  be  made  regarding  the  best 
time  schedule  for  treatment  (e.g.,  once  a  day,  once  every  48  hours,  di- 
vided doses  every  4-6  hours,  etc.). 

Could  side  effects  be  diminished  by  giving  smaller  doses  at  appropriate 
times?  Animal  experiments  have  demonstrated  circadian  susceptibility 
rhythms  for  bacteria,  drugs,  X-ray,  and  other  stimuli.  The  treatment  that 
kills  an  animal  at  one  point  on  his  daily  cycle  will  merely  make  him  sick  at 
another  phase.  Thus,  there  is  reason  to  believe  that  the  changing  state  of 
the  body  might  enhance  or  reduce  a  drug's  potency,  according  to  the  time 
it  is  administered.  In  June  of  1966,  Drs,  Falliers  and  Halberg  began  to 
study  a  time  factor  in  the  efficacy  of  hormone  treatment.  Between  June 
and  the  end  of  September,  28  CARIH  children  were  medicated  on  specific 
schedules  and  systematically  evaluated  for  the  crest  time  of  their  expira- 
tory flow  rate.  All  of  the  children  were  sick  enough  to  require  hormone 
medication  daily.  They  were  divided  into  four  groups  matched  as  closely 
as  possible  for  age,  sex,  home  locale,  allergies,  the  amount  of  hormone 
they  needed,  and  their  expiratory  peak  flow  rates.  For  4  months  they 
were  maintained  on  prednisone.  One  group  received  it  at  1  a.m.,  another 
at  7  a.m.,  another  at  1  p.m.,  and  another  at  7  p.m.  At  the  end  of  4  months 
it  was  clear  that  the  timing  of  the  hormone  affected  the  time  at  which  the 
child  now  showed  his  peak  expiratory  flow  rate.  These  asthmatic  children., 
who  had  been  very  similar  in  their  crest  phases  before  the  study,  now 
diverged  significantly . 

Youngsters  who  received  their  drug  at  1  a.m.  began  to  show  their  crests 
almost  2  hours  earlier  than  they  had  before.  Those  who  received  it  at  7 
a.m.  were  little  changed,  but  the  children  who  took  their  drug  at  1  p.m. 
now  showed  their  crests  about  6  hours  later  than  they  had  before.  The 
time  of  expiratory  crest  shifted  according  to  the  time  they  received  the 
hormone, 

Although  this  novel  finding  was  not  immediately  applicable  to  treat- 
ment, it  did  suggest  future  possibilities  in  the  scheduling  of  drugs.  The 
medical  staff  had  the  impression  that  children  receiving  their  medication 
at  1  a.m.  or  7  a.m.  were  clinically  better  off  than  the  others.  As  a  result  of 
the  study,  Dr.  Falliers  felt  that  the  dosage  might  possibly  be  reduced  if  it 
were  administered  in  two  segments.  This  procedure  is  now  being  tried. 
However,  it  is  still  too  early  to  discover  whether  the  timing  of  prednisone 
had  any  influence  on  side  effects,  for  these  sometimes  appear  only  after 
many  months  or  even  after  a  drug  is  discontinued.  The  youngsters  will  be 
monitored  for  side  effects  relative  to  drug  timing  throughout  their  stay  at 

461 


CARIH.  The  feasibility  of  such  long-term  surveillance  makes  the  institute 
an  excellent  focus  for  such  studies. 

A  clinical  study  of  about  2-y ears'  duiation  will  be  needed  before  the 
clinical  value  of  drug  scheduling  can  be  determined.  This  may  be  a  next 
step,  although  there  are  some  inherent  limitations  in  the  endurance  of  the 
children,  who  are  ill,  away  from  home,  and  undergoing  all  the  trials  of 
growth  and  of  being  young.  An  intermediate  study  may  ascertain  whether 
these  kids  can  function  on  smaller  amounts  of  steroids,  taken  in  two  daily 
doses. 

The  implications  of  this  study  extended  far  beyond  the  treatment  ol 
asthma,  for  steroid  hormones  are  staple  medication  for  many  illnesses 
whether  they  be  chronic  ailments  such  as  arthritis  or  ulcerative  colitis,  01 
an  emergency  crisis  such  as  pneumonia.  The  administration  of  steroids  is ; 
delicate  matter,  as  illustrated  by  the  kinds  of  serious  consequences  tha 
can  follow-the  development  of  diabetes  in  predisposed  persons,  or  th 
so-callecl  steroid  psychosis.  The  role  of  timing  in  the  regulatory  action  o 
hormones  makes  the  medical  substitution— by  regulation  from  without- 
far  more  complicated  manipulation  than  supplying  an  artificial  limb  to  a 
amputee.  The  exploration  of  this  factor  thus  has  ramifications  for  hclpin 
the  victims  of  a  great  many  illnesses. 

The  importance  of  circadian  rhythmicity  in  asthma,  and  in  undcrslam 
ing  asthma,  may  be  determined  better  when  round-the-clock  studies  ( 
physiological  functions  can  be  made  without  disturbing  the  children 
through  bio  telemetry .  In  Dr.  Purcell's  study  of  vocal  and  respiratory  b 
havior  via  FM  transmitters  worn  by  the  children,  there  may  be  a  fir 
indication  of  the  value  of  continuous  monitoring.  So  far,  at  least,  a  fir 
look  has  shown  that  the  asthmatic  child  may  have  different  timing  c 
specific  functions  than  the  normal  child.  This,  in  itself,  is  a  clue  and  ; 
encouragement  to  continue  examining  time  structure. 

Research  Grants.  MH  10385,  8415,  3269,  Also:  A  5963  and  Ch  5523 
Date  of  Interview:  Nov.  23,  1966 

References: 

FalHers,  C.  J.  Corticosteroids  and  anabolic  hormones  for  childhood  asthma,  C\ 

Pediat,,  1965,4,441, 
Falliers,  C.  J.,  McCann,  W.  P.,  Chai,  H.,  Ellis,  E.,  and  Yazdi,  N.  Controlled  study 

iodotherapy  for  childhood  asthma./.  Allergy,  1966,  38,  183. 
Halberg,  F.,  and  Falliers,  C.  J,  Variability  of  physiologic  circadian  crests  in  group* 

children  studied  "transversely."/.  Pediat.,  1966,  68,741. 
Hahn,  W.  W.  Autonomic  responses  of  asthmatic  children,  Psychosom,  Med.,  1  9(i6, 

4. 

Hahn,  W.  W.,  and  Clark,  J.  A.  Psychophysiological  reactivity  of  asthmatic  child 

1966  preprint. 
Jorgensen,  J.  R.,  and  Falliers,  C.  J,  A  rational  approach  to  corticosteroid  thcrapj 

asthma  in  children.  J.A.M.A.,  1966,198,773. 
Purcell,  K.  Critical  appraisal  of  psychosomatic  studies.  JV.  Y.  State  J,  Med,,  \  965 

16. 
Purcell,  K,,  and  Brady,  K.  Adaptation  to  the  invasion  of  privacy;  monitoring  bolii 

with  a  miniature  radio  transmitter.  Merrill-Palmer  Quart,  Behav.  and  Dcv.    1 

12,3. 

462 


investigator- 
Margaret  F.  Gutelius,  M.D. 
Director 

Child  Health  Center 

Comprehensive  Health  Care  Program 

Children's  Hospital 

Washington,  D.  C. 

Prepared  by- 

Clarissa  Wittenberg 

Increasingly  it  has  been  realized  that  helping  a  child  "in"  must  begin 

ry  early.  People  involved  with  Head  Start,  a  federally  sponsored  pre- 

100!  program  for  disadvantaged  children,  came  quickly  to  the  realization 

at  they  were  not  beginning  nearly  early  enough.  Some  suggested  expand- 

g  the  schools  and  preschool  programs  to  even  younger  groups  while  others 

It  very  strongly  that  the  young  child  should  be  assisted  while  still  in  his 

jme.  One  research  program  at  Children's  Hospital  in  Washington,  D.C., 

is  attempted  to  reach  young  mothers  to  help  them  learn  ways  of  caring 

jr  their  children  and  stimulating  their  intellectual  growth  so  that  they  will 

3  able  to  meet  with  success  when  they  enter  school.  This  program,  work- 

ig  with  black,  unmarried,  teenage  mothers  has  shown  that  many  can  be 

elpecl  to  provide  successfully  both  well-baby  care  and  the  sort  of  interne- 

ion  that  helps  a  baby  "learn  to  learn."  Results  show  that  babies  who  have 

.ad  this  combination  of  weli-baby  care  and  infant  stimulation  have  higher 

cvclopmental  I.Q.'s  at  6  months  of  age  than  babies  who  have  not  had  this 

u-ogram.  This  difference  continues  to  show  up  and  to  become  more  pro- 

lounced  when  the  babies  are  compared  again  at  1  year  and  2  years  of  age. 

Almost  all  of  the  mothers  in  the  program  had  ambitions  for  their  chil- 

Iren  to  "go  further"  than  they  themselves  had  done.  They  wanted  their 

children  to  be  successful  in  school  and  do  well  for  themselves  in  life.  This 

project  attempted  to  help  them  realize  the  important  first  steps  in  making 

these   dreams  come  true  for  their  babies.  Although  in  many  cases  the 

mothers  were  quite  immature  and  themselves  locked  in  cycles  of  poverty 

and  lack  of  education,  most  were  cooperative  and  eager  to  participate  in 

the  program. 


MOBILE  COACH  PROJECT 

A  total  of  140  young  women  expecting  their  first  child  were  found  at 
city  prenatal  clinics.  The  girls  selected  were  black,  and  between  1 5  and  1 9 

463 


years  of  age  and  lived  in  a  specific  area  surrounding  the  hospital.  The 
were  in  their  seventh  month  of  pregnancy,  of  normal  intelligence,  withoi 
chronic  physical  disease,  and  without  gross  emotional  pathology.  Tl 
group  was  then  divided  and  one  section  received  counseling  from  t( 
research  team  and  particularly  from  the  public  health  nurse;  the  other,  t! 
control  group,  received  prenatal  and  baby  care  given  by  city  clinics  ai 
city  health  department  nurses.  The  deliveries  took  place  in  city  hospifc 
or  in  private  hospitals  under  contract  to  the  city.  After  the  babies  we 
born,  a  second  screening  was  done,  and  only  normal  babies  weighing 
least  5  pounds  8  ounces  remained  in  the  study.  This  assured  that  t 
experimental  and  control  babies  had  essentially  the  same  characteristi 

Of  the  original  140,  95  pairs  of  mothers  and  children  remained  in  I 
final  study  group.  Twelve  babies  were  born  prematurely.  A  typical  rate 
prematurity  for  babies  born  to  ghetto  mothers  is  about  12  percent.  Til 
were  other  pregnancies  that  miscarried  or  resulted  in  stillborn  babies.  1 
odds  are  against  these  babies  even  in  cases  such  as  these  where  the  motr 
had  obtained  prenatal  care.  After  delivery  many  mothers  moved  away  i 
a  few  became  uncooperative.  Those  who  moved  within  a  half  hour's 
by  car  were  retained.  In  Washington,  this  encompasses  almost  the  en 
city  so  that  only  those  who  moved  to  the  suburbs  or  out  of  the  city  v 
lost  for  this  reason.  This  left  47  mothers  and  children  in  the  experimc 
group  and  48  in  the  control  group. 

A  mobile  coach  brought  well-baby  care  to  the  door  for  those  in 
experimental  group.  A  public  health  nurse  worked  with  each  mo  the 
dividually  to  teach  her  about  her  baby's  development.  The  mothers : 
encouraged  at  each  phase  of  development  to  respond  to  their  ba 
particular  needs  and  to  supply  a  stimulating  and  protective  envirorm 
The  mobile  coach  made  possible  a  very  active  reaching  out  and  facilit 
well  baby  care.  Those  in  the  control  group  were  visited  by  the  city  R. 
By  and  large,  the  city  nurses  only  followed  the  babies  for  two  or  t 
months.  However,  if  there  were  problems  it  was  up  to  the  mother  t> 
that  the  child  got  to  a  well-baby  clinic  for  check-ups.  All  mothers  it 
program  used  Children's  Hospital  clinics  and  facilities  in  emergencies 
at  times  of  serious  illness. 

The  orientation  of  the  stimulation  program  was  frankly  middle- 

and  its  major  goal  was  success  in  school  for  each  child.  This  was  a  pnu 

is  schools  remain  the  main  way  in  which  children  can  learn  the  t 

•>eed  to  know  to  be  successful  in  our  society  and  to  be  able  I 

1  good  jobs.  The  methods  taught  the  mothers  were  built  upo 

-*•  —  *_„ ting  and  expanding  the  child's  curiosity  and  develi 

n  was  to  work  with  the  mothers  until  the  child  rei 
lien  to  assist  the  families  in  placing  their  child 
Start  programs.  However,  Head  Start  programs 
3  3-year-olds  in  this  neighborhood  and  there  fort 
i  have  been  placed  in  preschools.  Some  success  i 
'ed,  however,  as  one  child  is  attending  a  Monl 
e  been  considered  eligible  for  scholarships  to  a 


private  school  in  the  city.  However,  it  is  almost  impossible  for  these 
mothers  to  provide  transportation  and  to  pay  even  incidental  expenses  for 
private  preschool  education  The  loss  of  the  planned  Head  Start  programs 
is  serious,  as  it  has  been  demonstrated  that  it  is  necessary  to  keep  up  a 
level  of  stimulation  if  a  child  is  going  to  continue  to  progress  and  not  fall 
behind. 


The  Mothers 

All  of  the  mothers  were  between  15  and  19  years  of  age,  unmarried, 
and  pregnant  for  the  first  time.  They  all  lived  in  a  15 -census-tract  area 
around  Children's  Hospital.  This  area  is  basically  a  low-income,  black 
ghetto.  It  is  an  area  of  old  rowhouses,  old  and  crowded  apartment  build- 
ings, a  high-crime  rate,  and  many  social  problems.  It  was  an  area  highly 
involved  in  the  riot  in  1968,  All  of  the  girls  had  normal  intelligence, 
between  70  and  115  on  the  Peabody  Picture  Vocabulary  Test,  and  were 
without  evidence  of  chronic  physical  disease  or  major  mental  pathology  as 
determined  by  a  short  interview  with  the  psychiatrist.  They  were  all  wil- 
ling and  able  to  participate  in  the  program.  Almost  70  percent  were  born 
and  raised  in  Washington.  All  of  them  had  finished  elementary  school,  and 
about  20  peicent  had  finished  high  school.  Most  had  received  average 
grades  in  the  regular  academic  curriculum  and  wished  to  finish  school. 
Many  wanted  to  take  additional  training  and  learn  some  skill  or  trade. 
Very  few  showed  anxiety  or  depression.  About  90  percent  were  pleased 
about  or  at  least  accepting  of  the  pregnancy.  Almost  half  had  known  the 
father  for  one  to  three  years  and  more  than  half  expressed  love  and 
confidence  in  him.  Most  wanted  to  get  married  and  have  one  to  three 
more  children.  None  saw  adoption  as  a  desirable  course  for  their  baby. 
Most  had  the  assurance  of  help  from  the  grandmother  or  other  relatives. 

The  girls  lived  in  crowded  households,  About  a  quarter  of  the  girls  lived 
with  both  parents,  but  for  another  half  of  the  group  their  mother  was  the 
head  of  the  house.  Over  half  of  both  groups  had  significant  contact  with 
their  father  or  another  man  who  had  helped  to  raise  them.  Sixty-eight 
percent  had  an.  adult  male  living  in  the  household.  Their  families'  income 
tended  to  be  at  the  level  for  unskilled  laborers.  However,  only  three  of  the 
girls  came  from  families  then  on  public  assistance.  Although  none  of  these 
girls  came  from  the  lowest  economic  level  or  from  the  most  disorganized 
families,  they  were  still  definitely  poor  and  lived  in  a  marginal  way.  Al- 
though all  of  the  families  said  they  served  meat  daily,  it  was  found  that 
they  lived  at  a  crisis  level  of  food  expenditure,  spending  less  than  the 
Department  of  Agriculture  1962  guidelines  for  emergency  minimum  ex- 
pense. Although  a  large  number  of  families  had  life  insurance,  almost  none 
had -health  insurance  or  savings.  Most  of  the  girls  came  from  homes  that 
were  characterized  by  good  housekeeping,  although  there  were  many 
basically  substandard  conditions,  such  as  shared  kitchens  and  deteriorated 
structures.  The  families,  by  and  large,  were  struggling  to  do  the  best  they 

465 


could.  In  all  of  these  respects,  the  mothers  and  their  backgrounds  wet 
essentially  the  same  in  both  the  experimental  and  control  groups, 

A  survey  was  done  piior  to  the  program  to  determine  the  child -rear  in 
attitudes  of  these  girls.  This  covered  the  entire  group.  Questions  wei 
asked  to  assess  their  attitudes  about  child  tearing  and  to  see  what  thi 
understood  of  child  development. 

The  first  question  dealt  with  the  handling  of  aggression  toward  tl 
parent.  The  girls  weie  asked  what  they  would  do  if  at  dusk  they  had 
biing  a  child  in  from  play  who  then  became  angry  and  kicked  and  \ 
them.  It  was  found  that  even  mild  aggression  towards  the  parents  is  n 
tolerated  and  is  usually  punished.  About  60  percent  of  the  mothers  wov 
respond  punitively  and  about  half  were  in  favor  of  spanking  in  such  a  ca 
Some  favored  explaining  why  and  then  spanking. 

Another  question  dealt  with  the  use  of  punishment  for  immature  1 
havior  during  toilet  training  and  was  concerned  with  the  acceptance 
slow  development  toward  maturity.  For  a  2-year-old  who  had  a  lapse 
toilet  training  most  would  scold  or  spank,  with  scolding  preferred.  Oi 
one  girl  would  ignore  such  an  incident,  whereas  most  middle-class  molh 
would  do  so. 

With  regard  to  diet,  most  felt  a  rather  active  controlling  role  was 
quiied  rather  than  a  passive  one.  Almost  all  of  the  girls  felt  that  (1 
would  coax  or  make  a  child  eat  vegetables  rather  than  simply  offer 
them  and  then  ignoring  it. 

In  the  crucial  area  of  verbal  development,  only  25  percent  of  the  g 
would  encourage  a  child  to  talk  when  adults  were  sitting  around  visit 
Many  would  permit  an  answer  if  the  child  were  spoken  to,  but  a  tl 
would  piefer  the  child  to  be  absolutely  quiet.  Here  the  girls  with 
higher  I.Q.'s  gave  significantly  different  and  more  positive  answers  t 
those  with  lower  scores.  This  encouragement  of  speech  and  the  acquisi 
of  early  and  basic  verbal  skills,  as  well  as  having  pleasant  associations  > 
speech,  is  extremely  important  in  the  development  of  comnuinicn 
skills.  The  investigators  noted  here  that  initially  the  mothers  were  pie; 
with  the  sounds  of  their  first  child  and  responded  naturally  with  co 
and  other  sounds.  It  was  found  that  with  help  the  mothers  continued 
healthy  trend  even  though  it  became  more  difficult  when  their  chil 
became  active  physically  and,  in  addition,  began  to  bombard  them 
questions. 

When  asked  what  they  would  do  if  a  child  was  misbehaving  all  day 
-  **  Question  designed  to  tap  the  girl's  perception  of  underlying  cans 


demonstrated  love  and  devotion  for  their  infants  during  the  newborn 
period.  One  question  with  regaid  to  what  to  do  when  a  child  was  aftaid  of 
the  dark  elicited  interesting  answers.  About  73  percent  would  leave  a 
night  light  on,  stay  with  the  child,  or  make  some  other  sympathetic 
gesture.  Even  girls  who  were  generally  harsh  about  discipline  seemed  lo  be 
understanding  in  this  situation. 

When  asked  as  expectant  mothers  what  they  thought  they  could  do  in 
the  first  five  years  of  the  child's  life  to  help  him  do  well  in  school,  many 
replied  with  answeis  such  as  "teaching  a  child  obedience,"  "teaching  him 
honesty,"  "name  and  addiess,"  "ABC's."  Only  about  a  third  said  "read  to 
him"  or  other  such  types  of  stimulation. 

It  must  be  remembered  that  both  experimental  and  control  groups  took 
part  in  this  study,  and  that  the  two  had  essentially  the  same  attitudes  and 
basically  were  the  same  in  approach  to  their  children. 


Prenatal  Nursing  Activities 

The  public  health  nurse  associated  with  the  research  visited  the  girls  in 
the  experimental  group  at  home  and  discussed  with  them  many  topics. 
Among  these  were  the  issues  of  diet,  their  pregnancy,  the  development  of 
the  baby  and  other  things  of  importance  to  the  girls.  Visits  were  made 
approximately  every  two  weeks  prior  to  the  ninth  month  of  gestation  and 
then  each  week  during  the  final  month.  The  nurse,  Mrs.  Marion  Brooks,  is 
black  as  weie  the  girls,  and  she  felt  that  this  helped  her  to  reach  them  and 
be  accepted  by  them.  She  felt  that  good  lapport  was  developed  with  all  of 
the  girls  that  she  visited,  and  that  most  were  eager  to  learn  and  com- 
municate with  her.  Her  general  approach  was  to  be  quite  definite  about 
methods  of  child  rearing  and  very  supportive  of  the  mother's  desire  to  do 
well  with  her  baby.  Her  visits  prior  to  the  birth  of  the  baby  lasted  about 
one-and-a-half  hours  each  time. 

Prior  to  their  deliveries,  the  mothers  were  encouraged  to  prepare  for  the 
birth  and  to  plan  their  care  for  the  child.  They  were  helped  to  collect 
bright,  colorful  pictures  for  the  walls  of  the  baby's  room,  and  the  nurse 
stressed  to  them  the  importance  of  visual,  auditory,  and  tactile  stimula- 
tion to  the  young  baby. 


Well  Baby  Care 

After  the  birth  of  the  baby,  a  comprehensive  program  of  well-baby  care 
was  begun  which  included  the  infant  stimulation  program.  Well-baby  care 
was  offered  in  a  dramatic  way  by  taking  a  mobile  coach  and  a  pediatric 
team  directly  to  the  home.  One  visit  was  made  each  month  for  the  first  six 
months  and  then  less  frequently  until  the  child  reached  3  years.  Between 
each  visit  the  nurse  visited  to  carry  on  the  infant  education  program. 
Although  there  were  some  technical  problems  with  the  use  of  the  coach, 
parking,  etc.,  it  was  considered  successful,  as  it  demonstrated  to  the 

467 


mothers  the  concern  that  was  felt  that  their  babies  receive  regular  i 
tion.  A  rather  large  coach  was  used  with  two  examining  rooms  £ 
toilet,  and  it  was  felt  that  perhaps  a  smaller  coach  would  have  been  j 
useful.  The  coach  allowed  the  home  examinations  to  go  on  in  a  p 
sional  setting  and  a  clean  environment  and  also  assured  that  the  staff 
pleasant  setting.  The  use  of  the  coach  has  now  been  discontinued  as 
of  the  children  have  now  reached  2  years  of  age. 

Medical  well-child  supervision  was  planned  and  administered  accc 
to  the  guidelines  of  the  American  Academy  of  Pediatrics.  The  progr 
infant  stimulation  was  developed  after  an  examination  of  the  lite 
and  a  study  of  Cald well's  inventory  of  Home  Stimulation. 

After  the  coach  was  discontinued  a  doctor  and  nurse  made  honu 
via  a  car.  The  visits  totaled  22  during  the  three-year  period.  The  ch 
in  the  comparison  group  were  followed  in  the  regular  District  of  Col 
health  clinics,  and  it  was  up  to  mothers  to  keep  appointments  and 
that  the  child  received  well  baby  care. 

The  research  families  were  counseled  at  each  visit  of  the  docU 
nurse  on  feeding,  hygiene,  sleep  habits  and  normal  growth.  Toilet  trr 
feeding  problems,  temper  tantrums,  thumb  sucking,  discipline,  and 
developmental  problems  were  anticipated  and  received  attention  pi 
and  during  the  period  of  their  occurrence. 

The  infants  were  provided  iron  and  vitamin  medication  in  proph 
doses  from  the  newborn  period  through  the  first  three  years  of  HI 
mothers  were  given  the  home  phone  numbers  of  the  pecliatricii 
nurse,  and  the  director  of  the  project,  and  were  urged  to  call  for 
whenever  it  was  needed.  The  mothers  called  often  in  the  early  mo 
their  child's  life  and  more  infrequently  later.  When  a  child  was 
mother  was  directed  to  take  him  to  Children's  Hospital,  althoi 
project  staff  treated  minor  illnesses,  such  as  colds,  diarrhea,  impetij 

Group  meetings  for  mothers  were  held  occasionally  on  Sundn; 
noons  with  the  project  psychiatrist,  nurse,  director,  and  occasiona 
to  discuss  problems  in  child  rearing.  It  was  observed  that  thclntct 
of  ideas  was  reassuring  to  these  young  mothers  and  stimulate 
interest  in  the  development  of  their  children.  In  addition  to  these  s- 
social  events  were  arranged.  One  such  outing  was  to  a  flower  sh 
another  was  a  swimming  party.  In  addition,  there  were  parties  held 
staff  member's  home.  Some  of  these  events  took  place  for  mo  the 
and  some  included  children.  These  were  designed  to  provide  c 
evidence  of  the  research  team's  interest  in  the  mothers  as  well 
babies. 

Some  of  the  mothers  had  a  feeling  of  being  singled  out  and  the. 
why  they  were  selected  while  pregnant  friends  had  not  been  in 
join  the  program,  They  were  told  that  it  had  to  do  with  the  timing 
pregnancy,  and  that  the  research  had  to  be  with  young  womoi 
seventh  month. 


468 


Infant  Stimulation  Program 

Aftei  the  babies  were  born,  the  mothers  were  visited  by  the  public 
health  nurse  and  a  carefully  designed  stimulation  program  was  carried  out. 
It  was  felt  that  the  very  best  teaching  of  the  young  infant  has  to  be  done 
by  the  mother  who  presumably  has  the  strongest  relationship,  and  who 
can  achieve  that  blend  of  intimacy,  affection,  and  stimulation  that  is  the 
key  to  a  responsive  baby  who  is  actively  able  to  learn.  Theie  is  an  assump- 
tion here  that  these  mothers  are  less  able  to  do  these  things  than  are 
middle-class  mothers.  There  is  also  the  assumption  that  these  children  will 
be  disadvantaged  when  in  school  with  middle-class  children  if  they  are  not 
helped.  There  is  concern  on  the  part  of  the  investigators  that  these 
mothers,  often  overwhelmed  as  they  are,  will  not  see  the  importance  of 
the  type  of  interaction  that  produces  an  open  and  curious  child  who  is 
able  to  learn  successfully.  The  stimulation  program  was  particularly  de- 
signed to  help  build  the  type  of  verbal-cognitive  background  that  typically 
occurs  in  the  middle-class  home.  It  is  thought  that  particularly  the  acquisi- 
tion of  verbal  skills  depends  upon  the  type  of  relationships  fostered  in  the 
early  days  of  a  child's  life.  Intelligence  in  this  study  is  conceived  of  as 
being  made  up  of  many  abilities,  many  attributes,  and  built  on  many 
expeiiences  and  supported  by  good  physical  health. 

This  program  was  designed  to  give  the  ghetto  mother  the  maximum 
suppoit  and  assistance. The  public  health  nurse  helped,  but  did  not  replace 
nor  displace  the  mother  in  her  role  with  her  infant.  Every  effort  was  made 
to  communicate  the  value  that  the  research  team  placed  upon  her  and  her 
infant.  Efforts  were  made  to  reduce  the  feeling  that  only  perfunctory  care 
would  be  available.  Many  of  these  girls  had  so  diminished  an  expectation 
of  assistance  that  they  were  slightly  suspicious  of  the  program  and  the 
quality  of  care  that  it  offered.  The  survey  on  characteristics  of  the 
mothers  and  their  attitudes  showed  a  basically  positive  approach  to  their 
children,  but  one  that  reflects  the  pressures  of  their  lives  and  also  their 
own  lack  of  positive  experience  in  many  learning  situations.  Although 
middle-class  families  do  not  always  reflect  the  most  sophisticated  ideas 
about  learning  and  development,  they  tend  at  least  to  have  a  better  idea 
about  these  factors  and  to  be  more  in  a  position  to  afford  the  type  of  toys 
and  to  provide  the  type  of  environment  where  a  child  can  at  least  explore 
ways  of  learning.  At  this  point  in  time  the  research  personnel  were  content 
to  try  and  achieve  a  little  more  of  the  middle-class  attitudes  towards  child 
rearing. 

Throughout  the  well-baby  program  and  specifically  in  the  stimulation 
contacts,  another  message  was  given  indirectly  to  each  mother.  It  was 
hoped  that  by  reaching  out  to  her  and  helping  her  care  for  her  infant  the 
importance  of  her  task  and  the  value  of  it  would  become  more  obvious  to 
her  and  that  her  own  early  sense  of  being  glad  about  becoming  a  mother, 
and  her  desire  to  take  good  care  of  her  child  would  be  supported  and  to 
some  degree  protected. 

469 


In   both   the  well-baby   and  the  stimulation  piograms,  as  the  n 
visited,  she  observed  the  mother's  behavioi  with  her  child  and  alsoij 
duced  and  discussed  various  issues.  Records  were  kept  via  checklist 
each  visit  in  the  first  three  years.  If  the  mother  was  not  home,  the 
taker,  often  the  grandmother,  was  seen. 

An  emphasis  was  placed  on  all  forms  of  visual,  auditory,  tactile 
motor  stimulation.  The  encouragement  of  exploration,  the  use  of  p 
the  understanding  of  normal  developmental  stages  were  all  impoi 
Avoidance  of  overstimulation  was  discussed  and  protection  of  the 
from  accidents  without  inhibiting  his  curiosity.  An  emphasis  was  p 
on  the  alleviation  of  superstition  and  on  assistance  to  the  child  in  har 
his  own  fears.  Normal  child  development  was  discussed  continuouslyH 

An  effort  was  made  to  see  the  mother  on  each  visit,  but  since  ma 
these  mothers  either  went  back  to  school  or  were  forced  to  go  to  ' 
often  the  baby  was  in  the  care  of  a  relative.  When  the  caretakers  we 
grandmothers,  they  weie,  in  general,  less  receptive  to  the  prograir 
were  the  mothers.  They  had  already  reared  children  and  tended  1 
back  on  ways  they  had  found  successful.  Also,  Mrs.  Brooks  fouri 
some  were  quite  superstitious,  for  instance,  fearing  to  let  a  baby  loo 
a  mirror  as  they  believed  it  caused  trouble  with  teething.  It  was  esti 
that  on  the  average  a  mother  was  at  home  with  her  baby  until  1 
about  1 5  months  of  age.  Four  of  the  mothers  remained  with  their 
until  they  were  3  years  old.  In  many  cases  a  good  day  care  center 
have  given  the  baby  more  stimulating  care  and  a  better  opportm 
have  the  type  of  experiences  that  would  help  him  cope  more  succe 
with  school  and  have  a  better  chance  of  breaking  a  cycle  of  poverty. 


The  First  Year  -  Stimulation  Program 

Ten  visits  were  made  by  the  nurse.  Each  one  lasted  about  1  ho 
nurse  gave  the  baby  toys  appropriate  to  his  age.  A  mobile,  a  rattle, 
squeeze  toy,  and  plastic  cookies  on  a  chain  were  given  to  the  ncv 
Later,  at  3  months,  a  cradle  gym,  a  terry  cloth  toy;  and  at  6  me 
cloth  book.  At  7  months,  a  plastic  hammer  and  a  plastic  milk  botl 
colorful  objects  inside  were  given.  A  stack  toy  was  chosen  for  9  i 
and  at  1  year,  a  more  advanced  cloth  book.  The  nurse  in  each  < 
plained  why  the  toy  was  appropriate  and,  if  necessary,  showed  the 
how  to  use  it.  The  toys  were  provided  rather  than  suggested  as  the 
gators  knew  it  was  difficult  for  the  mothers  to  afford  extras,  and 


helped  in  obseiving  how  the  baby  begins  to  speak  with  throaty  sounds  and 
then  progresses.  The  mother  would  be  encouraged  to  respond  to  the  baby 
and  be  told  how  important  that  was.  Trips  out  of  doors  and  to  new 
places  were  encouraged.  The  development  of  tactile  sense,  important  all 
throughout  life,  was  explained.  Cuddling,  stroking,  holding,  and  kissing 
weie  encouraged  and  the  mothers  were  warned  not  to  hamper  a  child's 
movements  by  more  furniture  or  clothing  than  what  was  necessary. 

As  the  need  for  discipline  grew,  the  mothers  were  helped  to  use  con- 
sistent and  patient  methods  with  an  emphasis  on  praise  and  the  under- 
standing that  a  child's  curiosity  is  valuable  and  not  "bad"  behavioi.  A 
frequent  problem  was  that  the  mothers  tended  to  confine  or  restrict  their 
babies  rather  than  safeguard  the  environment.  The  mothers  were  discour- 
aged from  slapping  and  from  continually  repeating  "no"  to  their  babies. 

The  Second  Year 

About  8  hour-long  visits  were  made  and  special  emphasis  was  put  on 
the  mother's  talking  with  her  child  and  encouraging  him  to  speak.  The  use 
of  books  and  bedtime  stories  was  stressed,  The  families  were  advised  to 
help  the  child  get  large  muscle  exercise  and  to  let  him  explore,  jump, 
climb,  and  otherwise  be  active.  Again,  as  in  the  well-baby  visits,  the 
subject  of  accident  prevention  was  raised. 

The  children  were  given  more  complicated  toys,  many  of  which  stressc-d 
fine  motoi  coordination,  such  as  wooden  beads  to  string  or  snap  beads. 
Balls  were  suggested  as  a  good  toy  for  helping  with  gross  motor  develop- 
ment. The  nurse  also  helped  the  mothers  to  develop  homemade  toys  and 
to  see  that  some  household  items  make  good  playthings. 

Although  the  nurse  encouraged  water  play  and  mud  pies,  etc.,  generally 
the  mothers  were  uncomfortable  and  felt  such  activities  were  too  messy. 
Generally,  the  mothers  did  not  want  their  children  to  get  dirty  or  to  put 
things  the  mothers  considered  dirty  in  their  mouths.  The  nurse  interpreted 
the  need  for  the  child  to  be  free  from  unnecessary  concern  about  tidiness 
in  order  to  explore  textures  and  to  play  in  an  imaginative  and  creative 
way. 

The  Third  Year 

Here,  there  was  an  increasing  emphasis  on  trips  outside  the  home,  on 
visual  motor  coordination,  and  language  development.  The  use  of  pencils 
and  crayons  was  encouraged.  Discussions  with  the  caretaker  or  mother 
emphasized  the  need  to  satisfy  the  child's  curiosity,  to  praise,  and  reward 
him  frequently,  and  to  admire  his  achievements.  The  mothers  were  en- 
couraged to  answer  the  interminable  questions  asked  and  were  helped  to 
see  that  questioning  was  important  to  the  child  and  the  basis  for  later 
intellectual  growth.  The  mothers  were  told  to  be  honest  with  their  chil- 
dren and  to  admit  it  when  they  didn't  know  the  answers.  Consistency  in 
discipline  was  again  stressed. 

471 


The  nurse  visited  about  six  times  during  the  third  yeai  and  each  visit 
lasted  about  one-and-one-half  hours.  Dental  hygiene  was  promoted  as  m 
the  overall  well-baby  progiam,  and  a  toothbrush  was  given  the  child  if  he 
didn't  already  have  one.  A  small  blackboard  and  chalk  and  eraser  were 
given  at  28  months  and  a  small  hardback  book  at  32  months.  A  puzzle  was 
given  as  a  farewell  present  at  3  years. 


What  Happened  to  the  Mothers 

Of  the  47  mothers  in  the  experimental  group,  17  have  married.  Out  of 
these  17,  five  have  already  had  separations  occur.  In  the  remaining  intact 
marriages,  most  of  the  husbands  work  at  semiskilled  jobs,  such  as  barber- 
ing,  electrical  work,  etc.  It  must  be  remembered  that  many  of  the  mothers 
were  still  dependent  upon  their  families'  support  and  that  those  who 
worked  generally  had  low-level  jobs.  Six  of  the  mothers  are  receiving 
public  assistance.  In  one  case,  the  money  goes  to  a  grandmother  who  is 
caring  for  the  child.  Of  the  experimental  mothers,  1 1  finished  high  school 
before  delivery  and  9  finished  after  the  birth  of  their  child.  Twenty-one  of 
the  mothers  remain  ungraduated.  Four  of  these  took  some  further  training 
after  delivery  and  four  are  still  in  school.  All  were  encouraged  by  the 
research  personnel  to  finish  high  school, 

Nineteen  of  the  47  experimental  mothers  have  had  other  children. 
Fourteen  of  these  had  one  more  child  and  five  had  two  more.  All  of  these 
mothers  have  been  given  access  to  birth-control  information  and  devices  as 
part  of  their  maternity  care  provided  by  the  city.  Of  the  mothers  who 
have  had  other  children,  10  have  obtained  consistent  well-baby  care  for 
their  additional  children. 

About  one-fourth  of  the  research  mothers  received  both  more  than  $50 
a  month  and  fairly  frequent  visits  from  the  alleged  father.  Another  one- 
fourth  had  visits  but  not  money,  and  the  rest  either  sent  small  amounts  of 
money  and  didn't  visit  or  else  ignored  the  entire  situation.  Not  nil  of  the 
mothers  who  married  wed  the  father  of  their  first  child. 

Most  of  the  mothers  had  enough  help  from  friends  or  relatives  to  leave 
the  baby  occasionally.  A  very  few  were  terribly  confined.  The  degree  lo 
which  they  left  their  babies  seemed  to  depend  upon  the  wishes  of  the 
girls.  Some  were  more  socially  active  and  felt  more  hindered  by  the  baby 
than  others.  Most  were  seen  to  be  caring  well  for  their  babies  and  demon- 
strated love  and  affection. 

Observations  of  the  mothers  and  babies  were  made  by  a  psychologist 
during  the  physical  examinations  at  6  months  of  age.  The  observer  was  un- 
aware of  the  groupings.  Results  showed  that  the  mothers  who  had  partic- 
ipated in  the  infant  stimulation  program  were  markedly  more  verbal  with 
their  babies  than  the  control  mothers.  This  has  not  been  completely  tested 
out,  but  appears  to  be  the  case  as  the  children  grow  older. 

Most  of  the  mothers  seen  by  the  research  team  were  positive  about  the 
program  and  made  contacts  continually  for  information,  in  addition  to 
regular  appointments.  For  the  most  part,  they  were  receptive  to  the  ideas 

472 


suggested  and  eager  to  learn.  By  and  large,  they  were  able  to  communicate 
well  with  the  research  personnel. 


Early  Results  -  The  Babies  at  6  Months 

Although  all  the  babies  started  out  well  at  the  newborn  stage  and  both 
groups  were  comparable,  there  were  differences  in  the  Bayley  test  in 
developmental  I.Q.'s  at  6  months,  and  at  1  year,  and  even  more  pro- 
nounced differences  at  2  years  of  age  when  the  experimental  children  had 
significantly  higher  scores  than  did  the  controls.  In  addition,  there  was 
significantly  less  anemia  in  the  experimental  group.  This  was  undoubtedly 
due  to  the  provision  of  iron  and  the  attention  given  to  the  diet  of  the 
children  during  the  "out  reach"  well -baby  care.  The  experimental  chil- 
dren, it  was  found,  had  also  received  more  meat  and  had  been  taken  out 
of  doors  more  often  than  the  control  children.  Both  practices  had  been 
encouraged  in  well-baby  checks  and  by  the  public  health  nurse  in  her 
stimulation  visits.  Babies  can  develop  anemia  very  easily  if  kept  on  a 
basically  milk  diet  since  milk  has  little  iron.  Their  bodies  need  this  sub- 
stance for  growth  and  development,  and  unless  supplemental  iron  is  given 
or  large  amounts  of  iron  rich  foods  are  added,  anemia  often  develops. 

At  6  months,  there  were  only  four  children  in  the  experimental  group 
with  hemoglobin  levels  below  10.0  gms.  per  100  ml.  Levels  below  10.0 
gms.  are  considered  anemic,  and  this  is  considered  one  index  of  general 
health.  There  were  14  control  children  with  levels  below  10.0  gms.  This 
was  a  significant  difference.  In  addition,  the  experimental  babies  had  less 
skin  trouble-primarily  diaper  rash-than  did  the  others.  They  had  better 
appetites  and  less  thumb  sucking,  too.  Although  it  might  be  predicted  that 
babies  in  the  experimental  group  would  have  fewer  illnesses,  this  did  not 
turn  out  to  be  the  case  and  patterns  in  this  respect  did  not  differ.  Growth 
patterns  were  not  significantly  different  in  any  way  cither. 

The  children  were  also  studied  intensively  physically,  anthropomorphl- 
cally,  and  in  terms  of  medical  history.  Again,  in  most  respects,  they  began 
the  same  and  continued  to  be  about  the  same. 

In  terms  of  the  mothers  and  their  family  situations,  again  many  factors 
were  studied,  such  as  crowding,  the  number  of  people  in  the  homes, 
incomes,  illnesses,  financial  resources,  diet,  and  education.  The  two  groups 
were  basically  the  same,  all  were  overburdened. 


More  Recent  Results 

The  Stanford-Binet  test  is  used  when  the  children  reach  3  years  of  age, 
and  to  date  a  trend  has  emerged  that  gives  the  experimental  children  the 
advantage.  This  is  a  statistically  significant  figure,  but  not  all  the  children 
have  reached  this  age.  It  appears  to  offer  some  evidence  of  the  value  of  the 
research  intervention. 

473 


Research  at  this  Point 


A  number  of  questions  are  being  examined  now.  A  large  number  of 
variables  have  been  recorded  and  a  statistical  analysis  is  being  conducted 
with  consultation  from  Arthur  Kirsch,  Ph.D.,  who  is  an  Associate  Profes- 
sor of  Statistics  at  George  Washington  University.  To  date  most  variables 
explored  have  either  been  unproductive  or  both  groups  have  remained 
very  similar,  except  in  terms  of  the  health  and  development  of  the  infants. 
A  close  examination  of  success  in  school  will  be  made  as  these  children 
begin  school.  No  attempt  is  being  made  to  modify  their  school  experience 
in  any  way.  The  relationship  between  the  mothers  and  their  children  will 
be  observed  and  to  a  certain  extent  the  relationship  of  the  mothers  to 
subsequent  children.  It  will  be  important,  if  positive  results  are  achieved, 
to  find  what  aspect  of  the  program  can  be  held  responsible  and  to  know 
which  aspects  really  work. 

One  major  problem  in  this  area  is  the  subtlety  of  the  results  that  arc 
being  measured.  All  of  these  children,  both  control  and  experimental,, 
have  received  well-baby  care,  in  fact,  the  D.  C.  health  department  R.N.'s 
who  do  this  also  have  a  certain  amount  of  infant  stimulation  education 
that  they  include  in  their  routine  contacts  with  new  mothers.  The  differ- 
ence then  is  one  of  degree.  All  the  mothers  live  in  crowded  homes,  all  have 
low  incomes,  and  so  on.  This  similarity  in  samples,  combined  with  the 
difficulty  in  finding  sensitive  evaluative  instruments,  means  that  differ- 
ences may  not  be  easily  seen  or  documented,  even  if  they  exist.  There  is 
considerable  conviction  on  the  part  of  the  staff  that  differences  exist. 
Some  differences,  they  feel,  are  related  to  the  relationship  between  the 
mothers  and  the  staff  and  the  enhancement  of  the  mother's  self  esteem 
due  to  her  participation  in  the  program.  There  is  a  growing  awareness  Unit 
the  large  social  problems  that  weigh  down  these  families  need  large  mid 
powerful  answers  to  go  along  with  the  improvement  of  infant  cure   in 
order  for   differences  to   be  seen.  One  idea  behind  working  with    the 
mothers,  rather  than  the  staff  working  directly  with  the  child,  is  to  give 
benefit  to  subsequent  children  as  well  as  the  child  in  this  research  pro- 
gram. Here,  again,  tremendous  pressures  make  themselves  felt.  Mothers 
who  participated  well  and  followed  directions,  who  seemed  to  understand 
the  value  of  well-baby  care  and  took  advantage  of  it  when  offered  by  the 
mobile  coach  team  still  do  not  always  take  subsequent  children  to  well- 
baby  clinics  in  a  consistent  manner.  There  is  also  some  concern    Unit 
despite  pressing  social  problems  and  the  availability  of  birth-control  infor 
mation,  these  young  women  seem  to  be  on  their  way  to  targe  families 
However,  it  is  possible  that  many  want  two  or  three  children  rclalivcl; 
close  together  and  that  having  achieved  this,  they  will  then  exercise  ai 
option  to  limit  their  families.  It  is  perhaps  too  early  to  tell  if  the  Idea  o 
birth  control  has  been  successfully  communicated. 


474 


Conclusions 

•  Early  results  indicate  that  young  children  can  be  helped  to  an  ade- 
quate level  of  verbal  ability  through  work  done  with  their  mothers. 

«  Relatively  uneducated  mothers  can  be  assisted  to  provide  a  stimu- 
lating environment  for  their  children  even  in  overcrowded  ghetto  homes. 

•  It  is  important  to  work  with  the  mother  who  is  a  key  figure  for  the 
baby,  and  she  may  be  able  to  use  the  same  training  with  subsequent 
children, 

•  Well-baby  care  will  be  better  utilized  if  taken  directly  to  the  home. 

•  Infant  anemia  can  be  wiped  out. 

•  There  is  a  dire  lack  of  preschool  facilities. 

•  There  is  a  desperate  need  for  good  day-care  centers  for  infants. 

•  A  nurse  who  has  been  given  only  a  small  amount  of  special  orienta- 
tion can  be  effective  in  an  infant  stimulation  program. 

•  Infant  stimulation  programs  should  be  incorporated  into  well-baby 
care. 

•  Infant  stimulation  programs  and  good  well-baby  care  alone  may  not 
alter  the  future  of  a  baby  born  to  a  young,  poor,  black  mother  living  in  a 
poverty  area. 

This  program  is  in  a  sense  a  minimal  one -relatively  inexpensive,  using 
limited  personnel,  and  the  extensive  use  of  a  nurse  to  provide  services.  It  is 
not  a  radical  departure  from  the  care  now  generally  believed  to  be  desir- 
able. The  teaching  of  the  mother  makes  possible  the  continuation  of 
the  spirit  of  the  program  and  hopefully  will  benefit  the  entire  family.  It 
appears  that  marked  changes  can  be  achieved  in  children  through  this  type 
of  stimulation,  and  that  this  is  a  fruitful  approach. 

Research  Grant:  MH  09215 

Dates  of  Interviews;  April  and  September  1970 

References: 

Brooks,  Marion.  A  stimulation  program  for  young  children  performed  by  a  Public 
Health  nurse  as  part  of  well  baby  care.  Paper  presented  at  the  American  Nursing 
Assocition  meeting,  1970,  and  accepted  for  publication  in  the  American  Journal  of 
Nursing. 

Caldwell,  Bettye.  Descriptive  evaluations  of  child  development  and  of  developmental 
settings.  Pediatrics,  Vol.  40,  July  1967,  No.  1 ,  46-54. 

Gutelius,  Margaret,  M.  Child  rearing  attitudes  of  teen-age  Negro  girls,  American  Journal 
of  Public  Health,  Vol.  60,  No.  1,  January  1970,  New  York,  93-104, 

Levenstein,  Phyllis,  Cognitive  growth  in  preschoolers  through  stimulation  of  verbal 
interaction  with  mothers.  Paper  presented  at  the  46th  annual  meeting  of  the 
American  Orthopsychiatric  Association,  New  York,  New  York,  April  1969. 

The  me  nobody  knows,  Children's  voices  from  the  ghetto.  Edited  by  Stephen  M. 
Joseph,  Discus  Book,  Avon  Books,  1969,  New  York,  p.  36. 


475 


Indeed,  what  is  there  that  does  not  appear  marvelous  when  ft  comes  to 
our  attention  for  the  first  time?  How  many  things,  too,  are  looked  upon 
as  quite  impossible  untff  they  have  been  effected? 


-Pliny  the  Eider 


Investigator 

Jerome  S.  Bruner,  Ph.D. 

Director,  The  Center  for  Cognitive  Studies 

Harvard  University 

Cambridge,  Massachusetts 

Prepared  by: 

Maya  Pines 


For  the  past  four  years,  Dr.  Jerome  S.  Bruner  has  been  concerned 
primarily  with  the  cognitive  development  of  babies:  How  does  the  human 
infant  -  so  helpless  and  limited  at  birth  —  learn  to  control  his  environment 
and  himself?  How  does  he  grow  up  intelligent? 

Although  human  babies  at  first  appear  more  stupid  than  chimpanzees 
of  the  same  age,  by  the  age  of  2  or  3  the  normal  child  has  achieved  one  of 
the  most  difficult  intellectual  feats  he  may  ever  perform:  he  has  re- 
invented the  rules  of  grammar,  all  by  himself,  and  he  has  learned  to  speak. 
He  has  also  constructed  a  fairly  complex  mental  model  of  the  world, 
which  allows  him  to  manipulate  various  aspects  of  the  world  in  his 
thoughts  and  fantasies.  And  he  has  learned  to  mobilize  various  skill  pat- 
terns whenever  he  needs  them.  Under  the  guidance  of  Dr.  Burner,  the 
Center  for  Cognitive  Studies  at  Harvard  is  trying  to  unravel  the  sources  of 
these  formidable  achievements. 

There  are  various  theories  about  how  children  acquire  language.  In  his 
book,  Verbal  Behavior  (1967),  Dr.  B.  F,  Skinner  claimed  that  children 
learn  to  speak  as  a  result  of  stimulus-response  conditioning.  All  compli- 
cated behavior  is  learned,  he  argued;  one  learns  to  behave  in  ordered  ways 
because  there  is  order  in  the  environment.  Linguists  such  as  Dr.  Noam 
Chomsky  disagreed,  suggesting  that  human  beings  have  an  innate  compe- 
tence for  language  which  sets  them  apart  from  all  other  creatures.  This 
innate  competence  is  what  allows  babies  to  learn  language  on  the  basis  of 
relatively  few  encounters  with  words  and  sentences,  they  claimed,  credit- 
ing babies  -  and  mankind  —  with  far  more  "mind"  than  most  scientists 
were  willing  to  accept  at  the  time. 


Innate  Competence 

Dr.  Bruner  and  a  number  of  other  psychologists  in  the  U.S.  and  abroad 
have  now  gone  one  step  further  than  Chomsky  in  emphasizing  the  im- 
portance and  activity  of  the  infant's  own  nervous  system.  By  studying 

479 


babies  well  before  they  learn  to  speak,  these  researchers  have  come  to  the 
conclusion  that  language  competence  is  just  one  example  of  an  even  more 
significant  ability  with  which  infants  enter  the  world:  the  basic  ability  to 
pick  up  logical  rules  from  mere  fragments  of  evidence,  and  then  use  these 
rules  in  a  variety  of  combinations.  There  are  programs  of  action  in  the 
human  mind  right  after  birth,  they  believe,  not  only  for  language  but  also 
for  the  intelligent  use  of  hands,  eyes  and  tools. 

"It's  a  very  different  view  of  man,"  says  Dr.  Brunei,  "and  it's  just 
beginning.  People  are  staiting  to  see  that  skids  of  this  wide-ranging  type 
couldn't  possibly  be  learned  element  by  element.  There  must  be  some 
kind  of  predisposition  in  man  to  allow  babies  to  pick  up  so  quickly  rule? 
that  go  for  such  a  large  number  of  situations. 

Only  a  few  years  ago,  it  was  generally  believed  that  newborns  could  no 
see  more  than  the  differences  between  light  and  dark;  that  during  thei 
first  three  months  of  life  they  were  so  absorbed  by  their  insides  that  the 
could  hardly  react  to  the  outside  world;  and  consequently,  that  the 
physical  environment  had  little  impact  on  them  -  all  that  mattered  was  ti 
provision  of  food  and  comfort.  It  now  turns  out,  however,  that  even  c 
the  day  of  their  birth,  infants  can  track  a  triangle  with  their  eyes.  By  0 
time  they  are  1  month  old,  they  can  spot  the  identity  of  objects  and  knc 
when  something  has  been  changed.  Furthermore,  they  actively   invc 
rules  of  theories  to  explain  what  they  perceive.  Even  at  3  weeks  of  age, 
infant  will  have  faiify  complex  hypotheses  about  the  world  he  has  ji 
been  born  into  -  and  if  he  is  proved  wrong,  he  may  burst  into  tears. 

That  was  exactly  what  happened  at  the  Center  two  years  ago,  during 
experiment   conducted  by  a   Radcliffe   undergraduate,   Shelley    Ros 
bloom.  Researchers  there  wondered  whether  babies  of  3  to  8  weeks  ret 
understood  that  a  person's  voice  should  come  from  the  spot  where 
person  stood  -  whether  babies  had  the  idea  of  a  locus.  If  so,  did  11 
babies  also  grasp  the  fact  that  when  the  person  moved,  the  sound  of 
voice  should  travel  along  the  same  path?  To  what  extent  had  they  orj 
ized  their  experience  at  that  age?  To  find  out,  it  was  decided  to  use  sl< 
speakers  that  could  separate  the  sound  of  a  voice  from  its  origin 
response  to  ads  in  the  Harvard  Crimson,  there  is  always  a  proccssio 
babies  ~  mostly  the  offspring  of  graduate  students  -  to  the  Center's 
where  they  are  made  comfortable,  given  toys,  and  usually  offered  st 
thine  interesting  to  see  or  do  as  part  of  a  psychological  experiment.  \ 


increasingly  differentiated.  But  there  is  some  notion  of  it  right  from  the 
start." 


The  Study  of  Infancy 

The  Center's  study  of  infancy  has  focused  on  five  issues: 

1)  How  the  infant  achieves  voluntary  control  of  behavior  in  a  fashion 
governed  by  prediction  and  anticipation; 

2)  How  visually  guided  and  intelligent  manipulative  behavior  emerges, 
with  emphasis  on  the  transfer  ability  and  generativeness  of  skills; 

3)  How  the  infant  progresses  from  being  a  "one-track"  enterprise  to 
being  able  to  carry  out  several  activities  simultaneously  and  under  the 
control  of  an  over-rule; 

4)  How  attention  develops  and  its  control  shifts  from  external  con- 
straints (novelty)  to  internal  constraints  (problem-solving);  and 

5)  How  pre-linguistic  codes  develop,  particularly  in  the  interaction  of 
the  mother  and  infant, 

O'Our  underlying  assumption  is  that  the  codes  of  language,  while  they 
may  indeed  reflect  innated  patterns,  are  first  primed  by  a  great  deal  of 
interactive  code-learning  of  a  nonsyntactic  type,'*  writes  Dr.  Bruner. 
"When  certain  of  these  pre-linguistic  manifestations  are  understood,  per- 
haps light  will  be  shed  on  the  deeper  question  of  the  nature  of  language  as 
such.") 

In  babies'  hands,  Dr.  Bruner  believes,  lie  clues  to  much  of  their  later 
development,  and  he  particularly  wants  to  find  out  how  babies  learn  the 
value  of  two-handed  ness.  Nobody,  teaches  infants  this  skill,  just  as  no- 
body teaches  them  to  talk.  Yet  around  the  age  of  1,  a  baby  will  master  the 
"two-handed  obstacle  box,"  a  simple  puzzle  devised  by  the  Center  to 
study  this  process.  Seated  on  his  mother's  lap,  he  will  suddenly  use  one 
hand  to  push  and  hold  a  transparent  cover,  while  the  other  hand  reaches 
inside  the  box  for  a  toy. 

To  Dr.  Bruner  this  is  extraordinary,  for  it  shows  that  the  baby  has 
learned  to  distinguish  between  the  two  kinds  of  grip  -  the  power  or  "hold- 
ing" grip,  which  stabilizes  an  object,  and  the  precision  or  "operating'*  grip, 
which  does  the  work.  Monkeys  and  apes  have  developed  a  precision  grip, 
Dr.  Bruner  says,  but  "it  is  not  until  one  comes  to  man  with  his  asymmetry 
that  the  power  grip  migrates  to  one  hand  (usually  the  left)  and  the  preci- 
sion to  the  other.'*  From  then  on,  he  emphasizes,  many  routines  can  be 
devised  for  holding  an  object  with  one  hand  while  working  it  with  the 
other,  leading  to  the  distinctively  human  use  of  tools  and  tool-making. 

The  experiments  at  the  Center  are  essentially  very  simple,  but  their 
interpretations  are  not.  Some  of  these  interpretations  parallel  Noam 
Chomsky's  "transformation"  approach  to  linguistics,  which  reduces  lan- 
guage to  basic  kernel  sentences,  each  one  made  up  of  a  noun  phrase  and  a 
verb  phrase.  Early  in  childhood  every  human  being  learns  the  logical  rules 
which  allow  him  to  transform  these  kernels  into  any  possible  sentence.  Dr. 
Bruner  speculates  that  when  a  baby  learns  to  differentiate  between  the 

481 


two  kinds  of  manual  grip,  this  foreshadows  "the  development  of  to 
comment  in  human  language"  -  the  basic  sentence  form  of  ; 
predicate,  which  may  be  found  in  all  laguages,  with  no  exception 
ever,  and  which  a  baby  expresses  when  he  combines  a  holophrase  I 
word  or  a  very  short  phrase  that  is  used  as  one  word)  with  anotVu 
Thus,  man  may  be  uniquely  predisposed,  at  birth,  to  reinvent  the 
grammar,  to  process  information,  and  to  develop  "clever  hands 
born  with  a  highly  complex  programing  system,  the  result  of  mi1 
years  of  evolution. 

What  about  disadvantaged  children,  then  —  why  should  they  t 
ent,  if  they  are  born  with  the  same  programing  system?  "Mind  ) 
can  ruin  a  child's  inheritance,  too,"  warns  Dr.  Bruner,  "with  an 
ment   where  he  acquires   helplessness.  You  can  also  be  traine 
stupid." 

Before  man's  marvellous  programing  system  can  be  activated 
guage,  for  instance,  a  baby  must  learn  a  series  of  primitive  cot 
these  require  interaction  with  an  adult.  "What  seems  to  get  es 
very  quickly  between  infant  and  parent  is  some  sort  of  code  o 
expectancy,"  says  Dr.  Bruner,  "when  the  adult  responds  to  an 
on  the  part  of  the  child,  thus  converting  some  feature  of  tl 
spontaneous  behavior  into  a  signal."  Right  from  the  start,  pi 
infant  are  busy  communicating  through  eye-to-eye  contact,  si 
sounds.  As  early  as  4  months  of  age,  an  infant  will  smile  more 
that  smiles  back  than  to  one  that  does  not  respond;  and  if  the  : 
then  stops  smiling  back,  the  infant  will  look  away.  In  some  case 
even  struggle  bodily  to  look  away.  A  child's  other  attempts  at  lej 
similarly  be  brought  to  a  halt  when  his  expectancy  is  thwarted,  i 
stop  making  sense. 


The  Development  of  Strategies 

The  prolonged  infancy  of  man  has  definite  functional  impor 
Bruner  concludes:  During  that  time,  the  infant  is  basically  deve 
strategies  that  will  later  be  combined  for  intelligent  action  -  f 
and  language,  as  well  as  for  the  manipulation  of  tools. 

One  such  strategy  is  "place-holding."  The  earliest  evidence  i 
be  seen  in  infants'  sucking  behavior.  As  everybody  knows,  a  p 
calm  a  baby.  But  why?  Earlier  research  had  shown  that  sucki 
hunger  pangs  and  relieves  muscle  tension.  "Well,  but  putting  ele 
the  temples  of  babies  as  they  were  watching  a  movie  here,  we'\ 
find  out  what  a  pacifier  really  does,"  says  Dr.  Bruner.  "One  c 
ipal  effects  is  to  cut  down  scanning  eye  movements,  which  cut 
baby's  information  intake."  At  birth,  and  for  a  few  days  therea 
can't  cope  with  more  than  one  activity  at  a  time.  When  they  w 
they  close  their  eyes  tightly,  to  avoid  taking  in  informatio 
outside.  When  their  eyes  are  open,  they  stop  sucking.  By  the  a 
weeks,  however,  they  can  suck  with  their  eyes  open  -  but  as  s 

482 


become  really  interested  in  something,  the  sucking  stops.  Finally,  between 
the  ages  of  2  and  4  months,  a  new  strategy  appears.  Whenever  something 
catches  their  attention  while  they  are  sucking,  they  stop  their  usual  suc- 
tioning  and  shift  to  a  sort  of  mouthing  which  keeps  the  nipple  active, 
though  at  a  reduced  rate.  This  allows  them  to  pick  up  where  they  left  off 
with  great  ease,  once  their  curiosity  has  been  satisfied-  A  neat  solution  to 
an  early  problem,  "place-holding"  of  this  sort  leads  to  many  later  skills, 
both  manual  and  linguistic. 

"As  I  got  more  into  this  work  on  skilled  behavior,  it  became  increas- 
ingly evident  to  what  extent  intention  and  hypothesis  are  central  to  the 
organization  of  knowledge  and  to  the  filtering  of  input,"  declares  Dr. 
Bruner.  In  his  most  recent  study,  "Studies  in  the  Growth  of  Manual 
Intelligence  in  Infancy,"  which  he  did  with  Karlen  Lyons  and  Kenneth 
Kaye,  Dr.  Bruner  emphasizes  the  importance  of  the  infants'  own  programs 
of  action.  "When  one  observes  the  early  behavior  of  infants  —  say  at  the 
onset  of  visually  guided  reaching  at  around  4  months  of  age  —  one  is  struck 
by  the  fact  that  arousal  of  intention  is  the  initial  reaction  to  an  'appropri- 
ate' stimulus,"  he  writes,  "The  earliest  overt  expression  of  activated  inten- 
sion is  not  'trial  and  error,'  but  an  awkward  but  recognizable  instrumental 
act  that  expresses  a  preadapted  progiam  of  action." 

His  movie,  "The  Intention  to  Take  -  The  Infancy  of  Object  Capture," 
illustrates  how  babies  begin  with  an  intention,  act  out  its  intended  results 
(or  an  approximation  thereof)  and  then  work  backward  to  the  compo- 
nents that  will  in  fact  make  such  results  possible.  "First,  they  look  at  the 
object,"  says  Dr.  Bruner,  describing  the  movie.  "They  want  to  take  it.  It's 
an  intense  gazing.  Then,  as  the  child's  intention  gets  organized,  his  lips 
come  forward  in  what  we  call  an  'A-frame  mouth.'  Later  on,  when  he 
takes  hold  of  the  object,  it  will  go  into  his  mouth;  but  already,  his  whole 
system  is  activated,  his  mouth  works.  Then  his  arms  come  up  in  an  anti- 
gravitational  movement,  and  up  comes  that  fist."  The  infants1  actions  are 
not  yet  in  the  right  ordei  for  success.  "Six  weeks  later,  these  actions  will 
seem  so  well  regulated  that  we'll  forget  the  complexity  of  even  so  simple  a 
task.  Then  they  will  leap  forward  to  a  fully  orchestrated  act.  But  the 
preparation  is  slow  and  demanding." 

The  infants'  own  intentions,  then,  are  crucial.  Of  course,  some  goals  can 
be  imposed  from  outside,  and  babies  can  be  taught,  for  instance,  to  re- 
spond to  a  buzzer  in  certain  ways.  Thus,  Dr.  Hanus  Papousek,  a  Czech 
psychologist  who  is  now  spending  a  year  at  the  Center,  has  conditioned 
newborns  to  turn  their  heads  sharply  to  the  side  at  the  sound  of  a  buzzer, 
in  order  to  get  milk  from  a  buzzer.  "It  can  be  done,"  says  Dr,  Bruner, 
"but  it's  endless.  The  babies  show  so  much  aversion  to  this.  They're  so 
slow  at  learning  it,  you  have  to  present  the  stimulus  hundreds  of  times," 

By  contrast,  when  the  infant  uses  his  own  initiative,  learning  often 
comes  with  lighning  speed.  In  the  Center's  lab,  a  medium-sized  room 
which  might  be  called  a  baby  theater,  babies  are  placed  in  a  well-padded 
seat  facing  a  blank  wall  which  serves  as  a  screen.  Then,  with  a  pacifier  in 
their  mouth,  they  are  shown  a  movie.  "We  didn't  want  to  condition  them 
to  respond  to  a  stimulus,"  Dr.  Bruner  explains.  "Instead,  we  wanted  to 

483 


choose  something  the  child  does  and  give  it  some  consequence.  Then  he  is 
at  the  controls.  So  we  chose  sucking.  Would  they  learn  to  suck  at  different 
speeds  in  order  to  produce  changes  in  their  environment'?  And,  lo  and 
behold,  these  little  4-,  5- and  6-week-old  infants  do  learn  to  suck  in  longer 
bursts  to  produce  a  clear  focus.  Or  else,  if  you  reverse  the  conditions  so 
that  sucking  blurs  the  pictuie,  they  learn  to  desist  from  sucking  on  this 
pacifier.  They  respond  immediately,  during  the  very  first  session,  to 
changes  produced  by  their  own  acts." 

The  movie  that  the  babies  watched  so  eagerly  showed  an  Eskimo 
mother  playing  with  her  child.  "It  was  shot  in  winter,  indoors,  and  she 
was  constantly  involved  in  little  games  with  him  —  string  games  and  so  on,1 
explains  Dr.  Bruner.  The  experiment  was  devised  by  a  graduate  student 
Mrs.  Ilze  Kalnins.  When  the  babies  discovered  that  sucking  made  th 
pictures  clearer,  they  cut  down  their  pauses  between  sucks,  stopping  onl 
four  seconds.  On  other  visits  to  the  lab,  when  they  found  that  suckin 
blurred  the  image,  they  lengthened  the  pauses  to  about  eight  seconds. 

The  babies'  performance  was  all  the  more  remarkable  because  of  the 
inexperience  with  "place-holding."  To  bring  the  picture  into  focus,  tlu 
had  to  suck  in  longer  bursts  without  looking  at  the  film,  then  take  a  quw 
look  before  it  blurred  again. 

Curiously,  this  experiment  comes  quite  close  to  the  kind  of  opera 
conditioning  pioneered  by  Dr.  Skinner,  in  which  rewards  ate  used 
"shape"  a  child's  activity.  But  Dr,  Bruner  interprets  it  quite  different 
seeing  the  babies'  rapid  learning  as  the  effect  of  fulfilling  their   o 
intentions.  Sucking  to  produce  a  sharp  focus  involves  quite  camp 
strategies  to  coordinate  looking  and  sucking.  Such  strategies  come  IV 
the  inside  out,  from  an  innate  preadaptation,  Dr.  Bruner  believes.  O 
after  their  appearance  has  been  evoked  by  events  can  trial  and  error  i 
reinforcements  be  of  any  use, 

"What  reinforcement  is  doing,  in  effect,  is  locking  in  that  response 
set  of  alternative  responses  which  in  fact  works/'  he  writes.  "It  docs 
bring  into  being  new  responses.  For  the  most  part,  the  children  do 
gradually  improve  their  strategies,  but  rather  increase  the  skill  with  w1 
they  perform  old  routines. ,  .  .  Two-handed  efforts  make  their  appears 
abruptly,  rather  than  by  some  gradual  route,  and  seem  to  be  'ready 
triggering.11 

He  points  to  an  experiment  performed  at  the  Center  two  years  ago 
three  groups  of  babies  of  different  ages.  The  babies  were  seated  in  fro 
a  table  on  which  a  jingly  toy  was  placed  behind  a  small  transparent  so 
open  at  one  end,  The  youngest  babies,  only  7  months  old,  simply  veil 
for  the  toy  with  the  nearest  hand  and  bumped  into  the  screen. 
banging  on  and  clawing  at  the  screen  for  a  while,  they  lost  intcrcs 
gave  up.  The  next  group,  the  1-year-olds,  began  in  the  same  fashioi 
then  let  their  hands  follow  the  edge  of  the  screen  and  reached  bchint 
a  sort  of  backhand  grasp  until  they  got  the  toy.  Only  the  18-nion1 
babies  knew  right  away  how  to  reach  the  toy  efficiently,  and  did  so 
1 6  trials  each,  none  of  the  babies  ever  changed  his  initial  strategy ;  th 
the  best  he  was  capable  of  at  that  stage. 

484 


"Trial  and  error  implies  the  capacity  to  hold  an  end  constant  while 
varying  means,"  notes  Dr.  Bruner.  "The  segments  in  which  this  is  possible 
are  very  short  in  duration  for  the  child.  What  thwarts  him  is  distraction, 
not  error."  This  is  why  he  is  so  interested  in  the  development  of  the 
child's  own  intentions,  and  in  the  kind  of  "planning  control"  described  by 
the  Russian  psychologist,  Dr.  A.  R.  Luria,  as  being  located  in  the  frontal 
lobes.  He  hopes  to  study  the  development  of  strategies  and  plans  in  pri- 
mates and  compare  it  to  that  of  human  infants,  so  as  to  gain  further 
insight  into  this  issue. 

Studies  of  Perception  and  Thinking 

The  Center  for  Cognitive  Studies  came  into  being  in  1960.  During  its 
first  years  of  operation,  it  paid  no  attention  to  babies.  For  Dr.  Brunner, 
too,  infant  development  is  a  comparatively  recent  interest.  Unlike  many 
other  psychologists,  who  study  the  same  topic  for  their  entire  working 
lives,  he  has  ranged  all  over  the  field.  And  before  calling  attention  to  the 
cognitive  giowth  of  infants,  he  had  helped  to  create  interest  in  four  major 
movements:  1 )  the  so-called  "New  Look"  in  perception  in  the  late  forties 
and  early  fifties;  2)  the  study  of  cognitive  processes,  mostly  in  adults;  3) 
educational  reform,  with  emphasis  on  new  curricula;  and  4)  the  study  of 
children's  cognitive  development,  Throughout,  he  always  came  back  to 
the  same  basic  questions:  How  do  human  beings  gather,  categorize,  store, 
use  and  communicate  knowledge? 

"You  can  never  get  a  direct  test  on  reality,"  he  says.  "You  must  take 
scraps  and  test  them  against  your  mental  model  of  the  world."  In  his  work 
on  perception,  he  wanted  to  learn  how  people  register  information 
through  the  filter  of  their  own  experience.  He  concluded  that  the  same 
objects  —  for  example,  coins  —  are  perceived  differently  by  different  people, 
in  accordance  with  their  values  and  needs.  "Perceptions  are  highly  regu- 
lated entry  ports,"  he  notes.  "An  experienced  eye  will  pick  up  so  much 
more!1'  In  contrast  to  work  that  considered  perception  to  be  strictly 
passive,  this  approach  was  called  "hot"  perception,  or  "the  New  Look"  in 
perception-  It  led  him  to  the  boundary  line  between  perception  and 
thinking. 

Together  with  other  members  of  the  Harvard  Cognition  Project,  he  then 
spent  five  years  studying  cognitive  processes  —  "the  means  whereby  organ- 
isms achieve,  retain  and  transform  information."  At  the  time,  this  was  a 
major  departure  from  the  accepted  approach  to  psychological  problems, 
behaviorism.  For  roughly  30  years,  most  positions  of  prestige  in  American 
psychology  had  gone  to  people  who  studied  stimuli  and  responses,  by- 
passing anything  that  smacked  of  the  "mental." 

Spurred  on  by  work  in  computer  simulation  and  information  theory,  a 
few  psychologists  were  beginning  to  worry  about  the  rnind  again.  Some- 
times they  called  it  "the  black  box."  Clearly,  the  black  box  had  to  sort 
out  all  the  inputs  and  outputs;  but  how  did  it  do  it?  The  behaviorists  did 
not  even  attempt  to  answer  this  question,  which  they  considered  irrele- 
vant. The  members  of  the  Harvard  Cognition  Project  did,  as  described  in 

485 


Dr.  Bruner  *s  book,  A  Study  of  Thinking  (1956).  Specifically,  they  tried  to 
deal  with  what  Dr.  Bruner  called  "one  of  the  simplest  and  most  ubiquit- 
ous phenomena  of  cognition:  categorizing  or  conceptualizing.  On  closer 
inspection,  it  is  not  so  simple.  The  spirit  of  the  inquiry  is  descriptive.  We 
have  not  sought  'explanation'  in  terms  of  learning  theory,  infonviiiliov 
theory,  or  personality  theory.  We  have  sought  to  describe  and  in  a  smwl 
measuie  to  explain  what  happens  when  an  intelligent  human  being  seek 
to  sort  the  environment  into  significant  classes  of  events  so  that  He  mtv 
end  by  treating  discriminably  different  things  as  equivalents." 

"There  were  some  strategy  theories  I  had  picked  up  from  John  vo 
Neumann,"  recalls  Dr.   Bruner.  "I  wanted  to  show  how,  in  problcn 
solving,  as  in  perception,  people  use  strategy  for  choosing  the  instanc 
they  want  to  think  about.  I  was  arguing  that  strategy  and  syslemat 
search  efforts  are  characteristic  of  all  living  systems  -  that  there  arc  sin 
lures  and  hypotheses  in  the  mind,  and  that  you're  constantly  testing  ttu 
against  fragmentary  evidence  from  the  environment.  You're  locked 
the  most  tragic  —  you're  locked  into  the  structures  that  are  spccics-spuci 
to  you,  because  that's  the  way  the  human  nervous  system  is.  But  over  u 
beyond  that,  there  is  a  way  in  which,  through  the  exercise  of  iniUalive 
your  part,  you  can  turn  on  your  own  infoimation,  reorder  it,  ami  puna 
hypotheses.  The  structures  in  men's  minds  are  productive,  generative,.1 
as  grammar  makes  it  possible  for  men  to  emit  any  numbei  of  ultcraiiu< 


The  Impact  of  Piaget 

The  emphasis  on  strategy  in  Burner's  work  on  peiception  and  Hio 
caught  the  interest  of  the  famous  Swiss  psychologist,  Dr.  Jean  Piagel 
was  the  last  tiling  he  expected  from  an  American  psychologist," 
Bruner  notes.  "I  guess  I'm  not  a  very  typical  American  psychologist 
least  my  colleagues  don't  think  so.  I  think  I'm  right  in  the  IradiUnr 
started  with  William  James,  of  pragmatism,  and  that  they're  very  imi 
the  tradition  of  Ivan  Pavlov!  You  know  -  'we  don't  have  to  look  insit 
organism,  there's  no  structure  at  all,  all  the  order  is  outside >  and  till  y> 
is  mirror  it.*  Well,  I  take  a  drastically  different  view."  Drs,  Bruiu 
Piaget  first  met  16  years  ago,  when  Dr.  Piaget  came  to  Boston  lo 
lecture.  And  Dr.  Bruner  was  among  the  first  Americans  to  nppreuta 
importance  of  Dr,  Piaget's  work, 

Piaget's  monumental  studies  of  child  development  had  been  i* 
in  the  U.S.  for  several  decades,  until  the  cognitive  movement  awake 
their  value.  In  bold  strokes,  as  well  as  painstaking  detail,  PmgcL  I 
scribed  the  growth  of  human  intelligence,  from  the  first  day  oT  IH 
adulthood.  He  had  shown  how  children  construct  their  own 
models  of  the  world  in  successive  stages,  following  an  invariant  se< 
though  they  may  go  through  the  stages  at  different  rates.  When  u  cl 
experienced  enough  conflict  between  reality  and  his  imago  ol 
changes  this  image  to  make  it  more  accurate.  Thus,  at  first  a  chikl 
understand  that  when  water  is  poured  out  of  a  full  glass  into  ;i  wit 

486 


which  it  fills  only  half  way,  the  amount  of  water  is  unchanged,  Being 
"centered"  on  only  one  aspect  of  reality  at  a  time,  he  sees  that  the  glass  is 
half  empty  and  says  there  is  "less"  water  than  before.  Through  a  series  of 
experiments,  Piaget  explored  how  children  develop  what  he  calls  "conser- 
vation," the  understanding  that  a  quantity  of  water  or  clay  will  remain  the 
same,  regardless  of  the  shape  it  takes.  As  children  realize  that  objects  and 
people  have  properties  that  do  not  depend  on  their  immediate  appearance, 
they  become  able  to  deal  with  symbols.  Intelligence  consists  of  such  leaps 
into  abstraction  -  but  it  depends  on  a  large  repertoire  of  images  with 
which  one  can  visualize  certain  sequences  of  cause  and  effect. 

Dr.  Brunei-  devotes  considerable  space  to  the  contribxUions  of  "the 
Geneva  school"  in  his  book,  The  Process  of  Education.  Many  of  his  own 
papers  show  a  strong  Piagetian  influence,  particularly  those  in  which  he 
discusses  the  stages  in  cognitive  growth.  But  eventually  he  developed  dif- 
ferences of  opinion  with  Piaget  about  how  children  acquire  the  notions  of 
conservation  and  -  much  more  fundamentally  —  about  what  produces 
intellectual  growth. 

''Mostly  we  argue  about  prefixedness,"  he  says.  "I  found  increasingly 
with  Piaget  that  his  notions  of  interior  order  were  much  more  prefigures, 
prefixed  than  mine.  I  think  this  was  the  thing  that  caused  something  of  an 
intellectual  rift  between  us,  I  think  that  he  misunderstands  me  more  than 
I  misunderstand  him.  He  is  too  concerned  with  how  the  mind  just  proc- 
esses things.  I  told  him  once,  only  half-jokingly,  that  his  study  of  mollusks 
(conducted  when  he  was  only  15)  was  characteristic  of  him.  His  idea  was 
that  there  was  a  mollusk,  and  no  matter  what  that  mollusk  ate  or  what 
that  mollusk  did,  it  always  turned  out  to  have  the  same  prefigured  shell. 
Piaget's  notion  of  intellectual  development  is  a  bit  too  much  like  his  early 
conception  of  the  way  in  which  a  mollusk  grows.  As  one  of  his  colleagues 
pointed  out  when  he  was  here  a  few  weeks  ago:  What  does  Piaget  need  a 
theory  of  education  for?  Either  the  child  hasn't  reached  the  right  stage, 
and  there's  no  point  in  trying  to  teach  him  anything;  or  he  has  already 
reached  that  stage,  and  why  bother  to  teach,  as  he'll  learn  anyway." 

In  Dr.  Burner's  view,  evolution  has  given  man  a  wide  range  of  possibil- 
ities —  far  wider  than  Piaget  would  allow  —  because  man  is  a  cultuer  user, 
and  his  growth  depends  largely  on  the  kinds  of  tools  he  uses.  "I  don't 
believe  you  can  or  should  separate  anthropology  from  psychology,"  he 
declares. 


The  Center  for  Cognitive  Studies 

By  I960,  a  number  of  converging  trends  made  the  study  of  cognition 
seem  particularly  promising.  Some  central  place  was  needed  to  stimulate 
interdisciplinary  research  on  the  subject.  With  grants  from  the  Carnegie 
Corporation  of  New  York  and  other  foundations,  Dr,  Brunei  then 
founded  the  Center  for  Cognitive  Studies,  together  with  Dr.  George  A. 
Miller,  a  psychologist  who  was  known  for  his  work  on  psycholinguistics  — 
the  study  of  how  cognition  and  language  interact. 

487 


At  first  the  Centei   focused  on  four  areas:  psycholinguistics,  h' 
memory,   perception,  and  the   cognitive  growth  of  children,  Amoi 
many  research  fellows  and  visitors  could  be  found  psychologists,  p 
ophers,  physicians,  linguists,  anthropologists,  sociologists  and  cyb 
icists.  Dr.  Burner  was  most  involved  in  research  on  the  cognitive  gro\ 
children,  particularly  those  between  the  ages  of  9  and  13.  A  Mobile  J 
atoiy  helped  him  and  his  associates  to  do  experiments  on  the  develo 
of  perception,  attention,  and  judgment  in  children  under  controlled 
lions,  right  next  to  the  children's  schools. 

In  the  meantime,  he  had  become  famous  in  another  field  -  edui 
This  helped  to  make  it  respectable  for  psychologists  to  be  concerne 
the  subject.  His  involvement  began  when  he  served  as  chairman  oi 
fercrtce  of  scientists,  scholars,  and  educators  at  Woods  Hole,  Cape  C 
better  ways  to  teach  science.  His  resulting  report,  The  Process  of 
tion  (I960),  was  the  clearest  work  on  curriculum  reform  at  the  tir 
won  him  instant  fame.  It  has  since  been  translated  into  22  language 
still  being  studied  by  teachers  all  over  the  world,  particularly  the 
statement  -  which  has  been  quoted  over  and  over  again  -  that  "any 
can  be  taught  effectively  in  some  intellectually  honest  form  to  ar 
at  any  stage  of  development," 

Although  many  of  Dr.  Bruner's  ideas  have  changed  since  then,  h 
by  this  famous  statement,  declaring  that  there  is  "absolutely  no  t 
against  it."  Another  dominant  theme  persists:  physics  (or  math.  It 
or  any  other  subject)  is  not  something  that  one  "knows  aboi 
something  one  "knows  how  to  do."  It  is  a  way  of  thinking,  rath* 
series  of  facts.  Thus,  when  Dr.  Bruner  devised  a  social  studies  cu 
for  the  fifth  grade,  "Man:  A  Course  of  Study,"  he  gave  10-year 
raw  materials  with  which  to  act  like  social  scientists  and  three  b? 
tions  to  start  them  off:  What  is  human  about  human  beings'? 
U\ev  set  that  way?  And  how  can  they  be  made  more  so?  The 
include  films  on  the  life  cycle  of  the  salmon,  on  free-ranging  babe 
on  the  Netsilik  Eskimos,  the  purest  surviving  example  of  traditu 
mo  culture  -  the  kind  of  authentic  records  previously  avanabl 
college  or  graduate  students.  The  course  has  now  been  adopted 
than    1,500  schools.  "Intellectual  activity  is  the  same  wheth 
frontiers  of  knowledge  or  in  a  third-grade  classroom,    Dr.  Brune 
Wto£l*tfBdu£tton.  He  still  believes  it  passionately.  And 
involved  in  the  creation  of  new  curricula  -  right  now,  a  new 
adolescents  on  principles  of  child  development. 

At  the  college  level,  he  proposes  a  dual  curriculurn  to  take  ad 
young  people's  drive  to  control  their  environment:  On  Monday 
days  and  Fridays,  students  would  continue  with  the  essential  bs 
sue'h  »  mathematics  or  language,  in  which  one  step  must  be  ta 
another;  and  on  Tuesdays  and  Thursdays,  they  would  be  1, 
gove  heir  own  learning  in  ways  as  experimental  as  possible. 
httUide  taking  part  in  budget  decisions,  teacher  evakiation  . 
TJ  more  than  that,  it  would  mean  that  they  coulc 


488 


own  problems  to  study.  Preferably,  these  should  be  problems  for  which 
no  answers  yet  exist. 

Students  are  usually  exposed  to  only  two  types  of  problems,  Dr.  Bruner 
points  out  those  which  require  analytical  thought  —  e.g.,  dealing  with 
abstract  formulas  -  and  those  which  require  them  to  do  some  kind  of 
laboratory  exercise.  "Both  are  formulated  by  the  instructor  or  the  text  or 
the  manual,  and  both  are  important  in  any  science,  art,  or  practical 
sphere,"  he  says.  "But  neither  is  much  like  problem-finding.  This  requires 
the  location  of  incompleteness,  anomaly,  trouble,  inequity  contradic- 
tion  " 


The  Growth  Sciences 

In  the  mid-sixties,  as  his  studies  on  children's  cognitive  growth  pro- 
gressed, Dr.  Bruner  became  increasingly  dissatisfied  with  the  age  group  he 
had  been  working  with.  "We  were  left  with  a  sharp  sense  of  incomplete- 
ness concerning  the  origins  of  what  we  had  studied ,'*  he  noted.  He  saw 
that  by  the  age  of  3,  a  repertory  of  skills  is  already  well  developed. 
Therefore,  he  began  studying  younger  and  younger  children. 

By  1967,  when  Dr.  George  Miller  left  the  Center,  the  transformation 
was  complete:  nearly  all  the  Center's  research  dealt  with  the  cognitive 
development  of  babies,  including  infants  only  a  few  weeks  or  a  few 
months  old.  Traditionally,  this  age  period  had  been  neglected  because  the 
child  seemed  so  inaccessible  between  his  fifth  day  of  life,  when  he  left  the 
maternity  hospital,  and  his  entry  into  nursery  school  at  3,  Dr.  Bruner 
urged  his  students  to  adopt  the  viewpoint  of  a  naturalist  exploring  a  new 
species,  rather  than  try  to  test  specific  hypotheses  derived  from  a  general 
theory  of  infant  development.  "Assume  that  you  are  studying  the  great- 
chested  Jabberwocky,"  he  advised. 

In  this  way,  he  took  a  lead  in  the  development  of  what  he  calls  "the 
growth  sciences,"  a  new  composite  discipline  concentrating  on  the  early 
years  of  life.  "Just  as  medical  research  was  organized  around  concepts  of 
pathology,  so  today  we  would  do  well  to  organize  our  efforts  anew 
around  the  concept  of  growth,"  he  declared.  "Those  sciences  that  can 
help  us  understand  and  nurture  human  growth  -  biological,  behavioral 
and  social  sciences  alike  -  should  find  ways  of  joining  forces  as  the 
growth  sciences.  Let  them  then  make  their  knowledge  relevant  to  those 
who  are  practitioners  of  the  nurturing  of  growth:  parents,  teachers, 
counselors.  It  is  bizzare  that  no  such  organization  has  yet  emerged,  though 
it  is  plainly  on  its  way." 

Research  Grant;  MH  1324 

Dates  of  Interview:  September  and  October  1970 

References: 

Bruner,  J.  S.,Goodnow,  J.  J.,  and  Austin,  G.  A.,  A  Study  of  Thinking.  Wiley,  1956. 
Bruner,  J.  S.,  Going  beyond  the  information  given,  in,  H.  Gruber  et  al,  (Eds.)  Con- 
temporary Approaches  to  Cognition.  Harvard  University  Press,  1957, 

489 


Brimer,  J    S.,  The  Process  of  Education.  Harvard  University  Press,  1960.  (Paperback 

edition,  Random  House  Vintage  Edition,  1963) 

Burner,  J.  S.,  The  course  of  cognitive  growth.  American  Psychologist,  1964,  19,  1-15. 
Brunei,  J.  S  ,  Oliver,  R.  R.,  Greenfield,  P.  M.,  et  al,  Studies  in  Cognitive  Growth,  John 

1  Wiley  &  Sons,  1966. 
Bruner,  J.   S.,   Toward  a    Theory   of  Instruction.   Harvaid  University  Press,   1966. 

(Paperback  edition  published  in  1968  by  W.  W.  Norton.) 
Bruner,  J.  S.,  Processes  of  Cognitive  Growth'  Infancy  (Vol.  Ill  Heinz  Werner  Lecture 

Series).  Clark  University  Press  with  Barre  Publishers,  1968. 
Bruner,  J.  S.  and  Bruner,  B.  M.,  On  voluntary  action  and  its  hierarchical  structuio.in 

A.  Koestler  and  J    R.  Smythies  (Eds),  The  Albach  Symposium  1968,  Beyond 

Reductionism,  New  Perspectives  in  the  life  sciences   The  Hutchinson  Publishing 

Group,  Ltd.,  London  1968 
Bruner,  J   S.  The  growth  and  structure  of  skill,  in  K.  J.  Connolly  (Ed.),  Mo  tor  Skills  in 

Infancy,  Ciba  Conference,  November  1968. 

Bruner,  J  S.,  Lyons,  K.,  The  Growth  of  Human  Manual  Intelligence:  I.  Taking  posses- 
sion of  objects,  in  preparation. 
Bruner,  J.  S.,  Lyons,  K.  &  Watkins,  D  ,  The  Growth  of  Human  Manual  Intelligence:  II. 

Acquisition  of  complementary  two-handedness,  in  preparation. 
Bruner,  J  S.,  Kaye,  K.,  and  Lyons,  K.,  The  Growth  of  Human  Manual  Intelligence:  111. 

The  development  of  detour  reaching,  in  preparation. 
Bruner,  J.  S.,  The  Relevance  of  Education,  W.  W,  Norton,  1971,  in  press. 


490 


Investigator: 

Roger  Brown,  Ph.  D. 

Harvard  University 

Cambridge,  Mass. 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 

A  number  of  ingenious  tests  for  showing  how  much  a  child  knows 
about  language  have  been  developed  by  an  NIMH  grantee  and  his  associ- 
ates and  are  being  used  by  several  investigators  here  and  abroad  in  work 
with  aphasics,  childien  with  retarded  speech  and  speech  disorders,  and 
children  who  are  deaf. 

The  grantee  is  Dr.  Roger  Brown,  professor  of  social  psychology  at 
Harvard  University  and  a  leading  authority  on  psycholinguistics,  or,  as  Dr. 
Brown  piefers  to  call  it,  the  psychology  of  language. 

This  investigator  himself  has  collaborated  in  using  the  tests  to  help 
determine  how  much  language-processing  ability  has  been  retained  by 
persons  with  brain  injuries.  But  his  goal  is  quite  different:  to  learn  how 
normal  children  acquire  grammar.  This  is  important,  he  points  out, 
because  speech  is  the  most  characteristic  human  performance,  yet  little  is 
known  of  the  mental  processes  involved  in  its  development. 

A  child  gets  his  first  knowledge  of  language  from  the  persons  close  to 
him,  and  he  tries  to  imitate  what  he  hears.  But  by  the  time  he  puts  words 
together^  he  has  begun,  apparently,  to  induce  a  sets  of  mles  for  their  use. 
Before  the  average  child  is  3,  he  has  somehow  developed  a  basic  grammar. 

The  child's  first  grammar  is  not  an  adult's.  It  leads  to  mistakes  like/ 
throwed  the  ball  and  to  peculiarities  of  word  usage  like  Kitty  (til  gone,  and 
it  is  far  from  complete.  However,  it  does  enable  a  child  to  put  together  a 
very  great  variety  of  sentences,  most  of  which  he  has  never  heard,  and 
thus  to  express  himself  on  his  own. 

Dr.  Brown's  group  is  attempting  to  discover  sets  of  rules  that  will 
produce,  in  the  sense  that  a  program  for  a  computer  will  produce,  the 
sentences  that  a  young  child  produces.  The  aim  is  to  get  behind  the  words 
and  the  sentences  to  the  machinery  that  turns  them  out.  Since  the  ap- 
propriate rules  would  lead  to  the  same  product  as  the  child's  brain,  they 
might  constitute  a  model  of  the  kind  of  operations  going  on  in  his  brain. 

Ultimately,  therefore,  this  research  may  add  to  knowledge  of  how  the 
brain  works.  By  elucidating  the  natural  language-learning  process  and  by 
demonstrating  how  many  children  know  about  language  by  the  time  they 
go  to  school,  the  research  may  also  lead  to  advances  in  the  teaching  both 
of  foreign  language  and  of  reading. 

The  investigators  use  two  general  techniques:  application  of  grammar 
tests,  particularly  in  the  case  of  children  older  than  3;  and  intensive  study 
of  language  usage  by  very  young  children. 

491 


Ill  some  of  the  tests  for  grammar  comprehension,  children  are  given 
nonsense  words  and  encouraged  to  use  them  in  speech,  with  the  investi- 
gators noting  what  rules  are  applied  in  forming  inflections,  like  the  plurals 
of  nouns  and  the  tenses  of  verbs. 

In  other  tests,  children  are  shown  pictures  and  asked  to  distinguish 
between,  for  example: 

The  dogs  dig  and  The  dog  digs. 

The  cup  is  falling  and  The  cup  will  fall, 

The  round  dish  on  the  table  and  The  dish  on  the  round  table. 

The  boy  is  pushed  by  the  girl  and  The  girl  is  pushed  by  the  boy.  (Chil- 
dren under  4,  Dr.  Brown  reports,  almost  invariably  go  wrong  on  this  one 
because,  evidently,  their  grammar  contains  no  passive  voice.) 

A  test  may  be  a  simple  direction,  such  as  Put  the  penny  in  the  glass, 
when  the  glass  is  standing  upside  down  so  that  putting  the  penny  on  it 
would  be  simpler. 

Dr.  Brown  and  his  cowoikers  have  found  they  can  use  such  tests  down 
to  the  age  of  3  to  learn  at  what  point  in  a  child's  development  the  brain 
processes  small  grammatical  differences  of  various  kinds.  Below  the  age  of 
3  they  have  to  depend  mainly  on  obseivation. 

In  studying  a  very  young  child,  the  investigators  visit  him  frequently 
and  record  his  and  his  mother's  speech  over  a  several-hour  period  of 
normal  activity,  The  transcription  of  the  recording  is  then  analyzed  anil 
rules  drawn  up  that  seem  to  match  those  the  child  himself  has  followed  in 
putting  together  two  or  more  words. 

The  technique  used  for  this  procedure  is  distributional  analysis,  in 
which  the  words  spoken  are  grouped  into  syntactical  classes  (on  the  basis 
of  where  and  how  frequently  they  have  been  used  in  the  sentences  and 
what  contexts  they  have  shared  with  other  words).  The  rules  take  the 
form  of  a  computer-like  program  for  generating  sentences  by  selecting 
words  from  the  groups  in  a  particular  order. 

The  investigators  test  this  program  by  using  it  to  tiy  to  duplicate  what 
the  child  has  already  said  and  by  noting  how  well  it  predicts  the  child's 
new  utterances,  during  later  visits,  using  the  same  vocabulary.  Preliminary 
work  with  such  programs  indicates  they  do  indeed  represent  the  grammar 
used  by  a  given  child  at  a  given  stage  of  development. 

The  first  children  studied  intensively  have  been  a  girl,  Anna,  who  was 
18  months  old  when  this  phase  of  the  work  began  (in  October  1962)  and 
a  boy,  Dale,  who  was  then  24  months  old.  They  have  been  visited  every 
other  week. 

Anna  and  Dale  are  the  children  of  middle-class,  well-educated  people. 
The  next  subjects  for  intensive  study  will  be  a  boy  and  a  girl  from  families 
considerably  lower  on  the  socio-economic  scale.  Dr.  Brown  reports  tlml 
such  children  lag  behind  others  in  every  index  of  speech  development 
because,  he  thinks,  there  has  been  less  interplay  between  mother  and 
child. 

492 


In  addition  to  collecting  more  information  for  a  language-learning 
model,  Dr.  Brown  wants  to  test  this  hypothesis  about  lower-class  families 
and  If  it  is  substantiated,  try  remedial  measures. 

#  #  :ii  #  * 

From  the  many  sentences  that  a  child  hears,  he  somehow  extracts  the 
latent  structure  of  the  language  and  for  the  rest  of  his  life  operates  in 
accordance  with  that  structure.  Dr.  Brown  and  his  associates  are  "trying 
to  discover  the  normal  progression  by  which  children  attain  to  the  rules  of 
English  and  also  trying  to  understand  the  learning  that  is  involved." 

Some  of  the  clearest  evidence  that  children  form  construction  rules,  Dr. 
Brown  observes,  lies  in  the  errors  children  make.  So  long  as  a  child  speaks 
correctly,  it  is  possible  that  he  says  only  what  he  has  heard.  But  when  a 
child  from  an  educated  family  says  "I  digged  in  the  yard,"  or  "I  saw  some 
sheeps,"  or  "Johnny  hurt  hisself,"  it  is  likely  that,  instead  of  imitating,  he 
is  applying  the  rules  he  has  induced.  (If  walked,  why  not  digged?  If  dogs, 
why  not  sheeps?  If  myself  and  herself,  why  not  hisself?) 

Other  evidence  comes  from  some  of  the  tests  the  researchers  have  used. 
For  example,  a  child  is  shown  a  picture  of  a  small  animal  and  told,  "This 
is  a  wug."  Then  he  is  shown  a  picture  of  two  animals  and  told,  "Now 

there   are  two  of  them.  There  are  two "  The  child  generally  says 

wugz.  If  the  animal  is  a  bik,  he  gives  the  plural  as  btks;  If  it  is  a  niss,  he 
gives  the  plural  as  nissez.  Thus,  even  with  words  he  has  never  heard,  he 
follows  the  rules  he  has  derived,  correctly,  for  forming  and  pronouncing 
plurals. 

When  a  child  begins  to  put  words  together,  generally  around  the  age  of 
18  months,  his  utterances  are  very  short-'*Anna  walk,"  for  example,  and 
"Dale  play  car."  Dr.  Brown  thinks  that  this  is  less  a  matter  of  limited 
memory,  since  the  child  may  know  several  hundred  words  and  be  able  to 
recall  them  when  needed,  than  of  limited  programing  ability.  During  the 
second  year  of  life,  this  usually  prevents  a  child  from  planning  sentences 
of  more  than  two  or  three  words.  (Of  a  dozen  children  studied  by  the 
Harvard  group,  those  about  2  years  old  had  an  average  span  of  2  words, 
with  a  range  of  from  1  to  4;  children  about  3  years  old  had  an  average 
span  of  5  words,  with  a  range  of  from  1  to  1  1.) 

Even  when  a  mother  asks  a  child  to  "say  what  I  say,"  the  child  reduces 
the  sentences  to  telegraphese.  "Mommy  is  going  to  have  her  soup"  be- 
comes "Mommy  soup"  whether  the  child  is  speaking  spontaneously  (using 
words  heard  before,  of  course)  or  in  immediate  Imitation  of  his  mother. 

In  the  beginning,  Dr.  Brown  reports,  the  child  probably  does  not  know 
that  in  reproducing  an  adult  sentence  he  cuts  it  down  to  the  most  signifi- 
cant words.  More  likely  he  selects  these  words,  mostly  nouns  and  verbs, 
mainly  because  of  the  emphasis  placed  upon  them  by  the  speaker.  The 
same  principle  holds  with  polysyllabic  words-when  they  are  beyond  the 
chikTs  grasp,  he  repeats  only  the  stressed  syllable. 

On  the  basis  of  the  work  done  under  the  NIMH  grant  and  previously, 
Dr.  Brown  describes  a  child's  first  speech  as  a  systematic  reduction  of 
adult  speech  marked  by  (1)  a  short  programming  limit  and  (2)  selection  in 

493 


favor  of  stressed  elements.  Features  of  the  child's  grammar  can  be  predict 
from  the  average  length  of  his  utterances.  The  investigators  found, 
instance,  that  children  whose  utterances  averaged  less  than  3.5  woi 
invariably  omitted  such  modal  auxiliaries  as  will  and  can  and  said  "I 
inside"  and  "I  make  a  tower."  Children  who  averaged  less  then  3.2  wo 
per  utteiance  always  omitted  the  foims  of  the  verb  to  be  in  progress 
constructions,  saying  not  "I  am  going"  but  "I  going." 

In  helping  a  child  go  from  telegraphic  English  to  grammatically  mi 
complete  speech,  the  investigators  note,  interplay  between  mother  a 
child  is  highly  important.  For  example,  Anna's  mother  says  to  li 
"Anna's  going  to  have  her  lunch,"  and  the  girl,  picking  out  the  stres: 
elements  parrots:  "Anna  lunch."  But  some  day  the  girl  heiself  ventui 
"Anna  lunch,"  and  the  mother  then  expands  the  utterance  into  the  m 
appropriate  simple  sentence.  In  one  situation  she  will  say  something  li 
"Yes,  Anna  is  going  to  have  her  lunch  now;"  in  another  situation,  "\ 
Anna  has  had  her  lunch."  Thiough  many  repetitions  of  this  sort,  the  cl 
learns  to  express  time-and  to  develop  rules  applicable  to  other  sentem 

One  way  to  teach  a  foreign  language  is  to  give  explicit  instruction  in 
rules  of  grammar.  A  newer  method,  Dr.  Brown  points  out,  is  to  treat 
student  like  a  child  and  have  him  repeat  sentences  over  and  over  agt 
Eventually  he  is  expected  to  be  able  to  produce  new  sentences-ones  t 
are  somehow  implied  by  those  in  the  practice  set.  This  is  essentially  w 
the  child  does  in  learning  to  speak.  Hence  studies  of  how  the  child  g 
about  making  his  new  sentences  may  contribute  to  the  improvement 
foreign-language  instruction. 

The  most  important  contributions  of  this  study,  however,  may  CQ 
from  the  increased  knowledge  it  piovides  of  normal  language  clevcl 
ment.  Arrested  or  abnormal  language  development  are  two  of  the  m 
prominent  symptoms  of  emotional,  mental,  or  neurological  disorder: 
childhood.  Understanding  the  factors  which  contribute  to  normal 
guage  development  may  serve  as  a  guideline  for  finding  the  factors  wl' 
contribute  to  the  erratic  speech  habits  of  the  emotionally  disturbed  cli 


Research  Grant:  MH  7088 
References: 

Brown,  R.  The  Acquisition  of  Language,  Presented  at  1962  meeting  of  the  Associa 

for  Research  m  Nervous  and  Menial  Diseases, 
Brown,  R.  and  Fraser,  C.  The  Acquisition  of  Syntax.  In  C.  Gofer  and  B.  Musg 

(Eds.),  Verbal  Behavior  and  Learning:  Problems  and  Processes.  McGraw  Hill,  1 

York,  1963. 
Fraser,  C.,  Bellugi,  U.,  and  Brown,  R.  Control  of  Grammar  in  Imitation,  Comprc 

sion,  and  Production,  Journal  of  Verbal  Hearing  and  Verbal  Behavior,  2:121-1 

1963, 


494 


Investigator: 

Richard  M.  Held,  Ph.D. 

Massachusetts  Institute  of  Technology 

Cambridge,  Mass. 

Prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


A  number  of  authorities  on 'child  development  have  reported  in  recent 
years  that  babies  raised  in  a  stimulating  environment  progress  faster  than 
other  babies.  One  of  the  first  experimental  tests  of  this  observation  was 
recently  made  in  connection  with  research  to  uncover  basic  mechanisms 
involved  in  the  development  and  maintenance  of  sensorimotor  coordina- 
tion. 

Shaping  this  research  program,  directed  by  Richard  M,  Held,  professor 
of  experimental  psychology  at  the  Massachusetts  Institute  of  Technology, 
is  the  idea  that  voluntary,  self-produced  movement,  with  its  accompany- 
ing feedback  signals  to  the  sensory  centers  of  the  brain,  is  highly  im- 
portant to  the  growth  of  the  sensorimotor  systems.  This  means,  for 
example,  that  we  can  reach  for  the  telephone  and,  without  fumbling,  pick 
it  up  because  we  have  had  a  good  deal  of  practice  in  coordinating  messages 
to  the  brain  from  the  world  around  us,  messages  from  the  brain  to  our 
muscles,  and  then  more  messages  as  the  muscles  act  to  change  our  rela- 
tionship to  the  external  world. 

Dr.  Held  and  his  associates  have  been  working  in  two  ways  to  demon- 
strate the  importance  of  motor-sensory  feedback:  They  have  tampered 
with  its  normal  operation  in  adult  human  beings  and  in  newborn  animals, 
and  they  have  increased  its  occurrence  in  babies. 

The  work  with  adults  has  involved,  typically,  the  wearing  of  prism 
goggles  that  make  objects  a  few  feet  away  appear  to  be  displaced  3  or  4 
inches— right  or  left,  up  or  down.  People  soon  compensate  for  the  dis- 
placement, it  has  long  been  known,  and  after  the  goggles  have  been  re- 
moved the  compensatory  effect  carries  over  for  a  time,  now  in  the  form  of 
error.  But  how  is  compensation  achieved?  In  a  series  of  experiments,  the 
MIT  group  has  demonstrated  that  it  occurs  when  what  the  individual  sees 
is  correlated  with  what  he  does.  If  he  views  his  surroundings  as  he  is 
pushed  in  a  wheelchair  instead  of  as  he  takes  a  walk  on  his  own,  for 
instance,  or  if  he  holds  his  hand  still  as  he  looks  at  it,  or  if  it  is  moved  by 
somebody  else  and  not  himself,  his  brain  makes  little  or  no  adjustment  to 
the  distortion  of  the  visual  signals.  This  work  seems  to  show,  then,  that 

495 


favor  of  stressed  elements.  Features  of  the  child's  grammar  can  be  predicts 
from  the  aveiage  length  of  his  utterances.  The  investigators  found,  fi 
instance,  that  children  whose  utteiances  averaged  less  than  3.5  won 
invariably  omitted  such  modal  auxiliaries  as  wilt  and  can  and  said  "I  ^ 
inside"  and  "I  make  a  tower,"  Children  who  averaged  less  then  3.2  won 
per  utterance  always  omitted  the  forms  of  the  verb  to  be  in  progress! 
constructions,  saying  not  "I  am  going"  but  "I  going." 

In  helping  a  child  go  from  telegraphic  English  to  grammatically  mo 
complete  speech,  the  investigators  note,  interplay  between  mother  ai 
child  is  highly  important.  Foi  example,  Anna's  mother  says  to  IK 
"Anna's  going  to  have  hei  lunch,"  and  the  girl,  picking  out  the  stressi 
elements  parrots:  "Anna  lunch."  But  some  day  the  girl  herself  venture 
"Anna  lunch,"  and  the  mother  then  expands  the  utterance  into  the  mo 
appropriate  simple  sentence.  In  one  situation  she  will  say  something  lik 
"Yes,  Anna  is  going  to  have  her  lunch  now;"  in  another  situation,  "Yt 
Anna  has  had  her  lunch."  Thiough  many  repetitions  of  this  sort,  the  chi 
learns  to  express  time-and  to  develop  rules  applicable  to  other  sentence 

One  way  to  teach  a  foreign  language  is  to  give  explicit  instruction  in  t 
rules  of  grammar.  A  newer  method,  Dr.  Brown  points  out,  is  to  treat  t 
student  like  a  child  and  have  Win  repeat  sentences  over  and  over  agai 
Eventually  he  is  expected  to  be  able  to  produce  new  sentences-ones  tli 
are  somehow  implied  by  those  in  the  practice  set.  This  is  essentially  wh 
the  child  does  in  learning  to  speak.  Hence  studies  of  how  the  child  gc 
about  making  his  new  sentences  may  contribute  to  the  improvement 
foreign-language  instiuction. 

The  most  important  contributions  of  this  study,  however,  may  cot 
from  the  increased  knowledge  it  provides  of  normal  language  devclc 
ment.  Arrested  or  abnoimal  language  development  are  two  of  the  me 
prominent  symptoms  of  emotional,  mental,  or  neurological  disorders 
childhood.  Understanding  the  factors  which  contribute  to  normal  h 
guage  development  may  serve  as  a  guideline  for  finding  the  factors  whi 
contribute  to  the  erratic  speech  habits  of  the  emotionally  disturbed  chi 


Research  Grant:  MH  7088 
References' 

Brown,  R.  The  Acquisition  of  Language .  Presented  at  1962  meeting  of  the  Associnti 

for  Research  in  Nervous  and  Mental  Diseases, 
Brown,  R.  and  Fraser,  C.  The  Acquisition  of  Syntax.  In  C.  Cofer  and  B.  Musgn 

(Eds.),  Verbal  Behavior  and  Learning:  Problems  and  Processes,  McGraw  1 1111.  N 

York,  1963, 
Fraser,  C  ,  Belhigi,  U.,  and  Brown,  R.  Control  of  Grammar  in  Imitation,  Compreh 

sion,  and  Production,  Journal  of  Verbal  Hearing  and  Verbal  Behavior,  2:121-1' 

1963, 


494 


Investigator: 

Richard  M.  Held,  Ph.D. 

Massachusetts  Institute  of  Technology 

Cambridge,  Mass. 

Prepared  by. 

Herbert  Yahraes 


A  number  of  authorities  on  child  development  have  reported  in  recent 
years  that  babies  raised  in  a  stimulating  environment  progress  faster  than 
other  babies.  One  of  the  first  experimental  tests  of  this  observation  was 
recently  made  in  connection  with  research  to  uncover  basic  mechanisms 
involved  in  the  development  and  maintenance  of  sensorimotor  coordina- 
tion. 

Shaping  this  research  program,  directed  by  Richard  M.  Held,  professor 
of  experimental  psychology  at  the  Massachusetts  Institute  of  Technology, 
is  the  idea  that  voluntary,  self-produced  movement,  with  its  accompany- 
ing feedback  signals  to  the  sensory  centers  of  the  brain,  is  highly  im- 
portant to  the  growth  of  the  sensorimotor  systems.  This  means,  for 
example,  that  we  can  reach  for  the  telephone  and,  without  fumbling,  pick 
it  up  because  we  have  had  a  good  deal  of  practice  in  coordinating  messages 
to  the  brain  from  the  world  around  us,  messages  from  the  brain  to  our 
muscles,  and  then  more  messages  as  the  muscles  act  to  change  our  rela- 
tionship to  the  external  world. 

Dr.  Held  and  his  associates  have  been  working  in  two  ways  to  demon- 
strate the  importance  of  motor-sensory  feedback:  They  have  tampered 
with  its  normal  operation  in  adult  human  beings  and  in  newborn  animals, 
and  they  have  increased  its  occurrence  in  babies. 

The  work  with  adults  has  involved,  typically,  the  wearing  of  prism 
goggles  that  make  objects  a  few  feet  away  appear  to  be  displaced  3  or  4 
inches—right  or  left,  up  or  down.  People  soon  compensate  for  the  dis- 
placement, it  has  long  been  known,  and  after  the  goggles  have  been  re- 
moved the  compensatory  effect  carries  over  for  a  time,  now  in  the  form  of 
error.  But  how  is  compensation  achieved?  In  a  series  of  experiments,  the 
MIT  group  has  demonstrated  that  it  occurs  when  what  the  individual  sees 
is  correlated  with  what  he  does.  If  he  views  his  surroundings  as  he  is 
pushed  in  a  wheelchair  instead  of  as  he  takes  a  walk  on  his  own,  for 
instance,  or  if  he  holds  his  hand  still  as  he  looks  at  it,  or  if  it  is  moved  by 
somebody  else  and  not  himself,  his  brain  makes  little  or  no  adjustment  to 
the  distortion  of  the  visual  signals.  This  work  seems  to  show,  then,  that 


compensation  depends  to  an  important  extent  upon  the  feedback  that 
accompanies  voluntary  movement. 

Other  conditions  besides  active  movement  together  with  its  sensory 
feedback  will  produce  some  change  that  may  be  called  adaptation  or 
compensation.  This  is  particularly  the  case  when  a  person  can  gain  infor- 
mation about  the  errois  he  makes  while  wearing  prisms.  He  can  then  note 
his  mistakes  and  very  quickly  correct  them.  The  MIT  researchers  believe, 
however,  that  this  kind  of  correction  involves  a  rather  different  sort  of 
process  from  the  one  they  have  been  mainly  concerned  with,  which  seems 
applicable  to  questions  about  the  very  early  development  of  sensorimotor 
control. 

Though  the  experiments  with  prism  goggles  may  superficially  appear 
rather  strange  and  even  bizarre,  they  are  considered  quite  relevant  to  a 
normally  occurring  process.  During  growth,  Dr.  Held  explains,  head  size 
increases  until  its  linear  dimensions  become  at  least  one  and  a  half  times  as 
large  as  at  birth.  As  that  happens,  the  distance  between  the  eyes  grows, 
the  distance  between  the  ears  grows,  and  other  parts  of  the  sensory  system 
change.  Because  of  these  anatomical  changes,  the  adult  and  the  young  child 
will  have  different  sensoiy  inputs  for  the  same  external  conditions.  The 
fact  that  the  adult  can  handle  the  same  sensorimotor  tasks  as  the  child 
indicates  that  some  adjustment  must  have  taken  place.  Further,  the 
optical  properties  of  the  eyes  change,  too,  in  the  course  of  a  lifetime, 
and  these  changes  may  require  compensation.  "In  our  experiments  the 
changes  are  abrupt,"  says  the  investigator,  "while  in  the  natural  course  of 
things  they  are  very  slow,  but  we  think  that  some  of  the  factors  affecting 
adaptation  are  probably  the  same." 

In  the  work  with  babies,  done  under  the  immediate  supervision  of  a 
psychologist  named  Burton  White  who  is  particularly  interested  in  child 
development,  the  researchers  studied  several  groups  of  infants  who  had 
been  born  and  were  being  reared  in  an  institution,  A  control  group  got  the 
institution's  usual  care,  which  was  adequate  for  general  health  but  pro- 
vided a  minimum  of  handling  and  other  stimulation.  Other  babies  were 
given  20  minutes'  extra  handling  every  day  for  a  month,  beginning  when 
they  were  6  days  old.  Up  to  their  fifth  month,  at  least,  these  handled 
babies  were  significantly  more  visually  attentive-shifted  their  gaze  more 
often  from  one  thing  to  another-than  the  controls.  But  there  was  no 
other  apparent  change. 

Another  group  of  babies  who  had  received  the  extra  handling  were  then 
given  a  better-than-usual  opportunity  to  move  in  their  cribs  and  to  look  at 
things  more  interesting  than  the  monotone  ceiling  and  the  white  sheets 
and  crib  liners.  The  mattresses  were  flattened  so  that  the  babies,  instead  of 
lying  in  a  trough  all  day,  could  roll  a  little  and  more  readily  move  head, 
arms,  and  trunk.  Three  times  a  day,  for  15  minutes  at  a  time,  the  babies 
were  placed  on  their  stomachs  and  the  crib  liners  removed,  thus  encourag- 
ing the  children  to  raise  their  heads  and  make  other  movements  in  order 
to  watch  the  ward  activities.  Interesting  objects  were  hung  where  the 
babies  could  see  and  swipe  at  them. 

496 


One  big  change  occurred.  At  the  median  age  of  3  months,  these  chil- 
dren began  to  reach  for  and  grasp  objects  effectively;  the  control  group 
needed  5  months.  The  results  are  what  would  have  been  predicted,  the 
investigators  point  out,  from  the  theory  that  the  development  of  sen- 
sorimotor coordination  depends  to  a  great  extent  upon  self-initiated 
movement  and  the  accompanying  feedback  to  the  senses.  At  least  as  far  as 
the  ability  to  reach  and  grasp  is  concerned,  a  baby  apparently  develops 
faster  when  he  has  an  opportunity  to  move  and  visually  interesting  sur- 
roundings to  encourage  him  to  move. 

Experiments  by  Dr.  Held  and  Dr.  Alan  Hein  with  cats  and  by  Dr.  Held 
and  Joseph  A.  Bauer,  Jr.,  with  monkeys  tend  to  support  the  same  theory, 
Kittens  fitted  in  infancy  with  ruffs  that  prevented  them  from  seeing  their 
paws  were  unable  a  few  weeks  later,  when  the  ruffs  had  been  lemoved,  to 
guide  their  paws  on  the  basis  of  vision.  Time  after  time  these  kittens 
would  pounce  on  a  slowly  swinging  ball  and  miss  it.  Much  the  same  thing 
happened  with  monkeys  raised  for  some  weeks  in  a  "holder  chair1'  so 
constructed  that  they  could  use  their  hands  but  could  not  see  them.  When 
they  were  finally  allowed  to  sec  one  hand,  they  could  hardly  keep  their 
eyes  from  it,  and  the  experimenters  had  difficulty  enticing  them  to  reach 
for  food  or  toys.  When  the  animals  did  reach,  they  usually  missed.  Only 
when  they  had  seen  the  hand  a  long  time,  from  10  to  20  hours,  did  they 
bring  the  movements  under  control.  Allowed  then  to  see  the  other  hand, 
the  monkeys  were  just  as  surprised— and  just  as  unable  to  control  its 
movements  to  get  what  they  wanted. 

Dr.  Held  points  out  that  persons  who  have  been  blind  since  birth  do 
achieve  sensorimotor  coordination.  In  this  case  the  sensory  feedback  that 
accompanies  movement  is  delivered  by  the  senses  of  touch  and  hearing. 
Every  once  in  a  while  a  person  long  blind  recovers  his  sight  and  then  finds 
considerable  difficulty,  for  a  time,  in  coordinating  what  he  sees  with  what 
he  does.  He  reaches  for  a  cup  of  coffee  and,  because  he  has  had  no 
opportunity  to  maintain  motor-visual  coordination,  upsets  it. 

Besides  increasing  our  basic  knowledge  of  how  the  central  nervous 
system  operates,  the  MIT  research  may  have  results  of  immediately  prac- 
tical importance.  The  work  with  babies  has  already  provided  scientific 
evidence,  the  investigators  point  out,  that  certain  kinds  of  experience  veiy 
early  in  life  lead  to  increased  alertness  and  faster  visual-motor  develop- 
ment. And  authorities  on  child  development  believe  that  the  growth  of 
sensorimotor  capacities  leads  to  the  growth  of  intellectual  capacity.  The 
findings  and  prospective  findings  may  also  throw  some  light  on  the  devel- 
opment of  certain  visual  disturbances,  notably  strabismus  and  amblyopia. 
The  research  may  have  a  suggestion,  too,  for  rehabilitation  workers. 
Patients  long  bedridden  lose  something  of  their  ability  to  control  the 
movements  of  their  arms  and  legs.  This  is  less  a  matter  of  muscular  weak- 
ness, Dr.  Held  suspects,  than  a  failure  to  have  performed  those  movements 
and  thus  to  have  obtained  the  feedback  from  them— conditions  that  seem 
to  be  important  to  the  maintenance  of  control. 

Voluntary  movement  and  the  accompanying  sensory  feedback  are  im- 
portant, the  investigator  sums  up, 

497 


compensation  depends  to  an  important  extent  upon  the  feedback  that 
accompanies  voluntary  movement. 

Other  conditions  besides  active  movement  together  with  its  sensory 
feedback  will  produce  some  change  that  may  be  called  adaptation  or 
compensation.  This  is  particularly  the  case  when  a  person  can  gain  infor- 
mation about  the  errors  he  makes  while  wearing  prisms.  He  can  then  note 
his  mistakes  and  very  quickly  correct  them.  The  MIT  researches  believe, 
however,  that  this  kind  of  correction  involves  a  rather  different  sort  of 
process  from  the  one  they  have  been  mainly  concerned  with,  which  seems 
applicable  to  questions  about  the  very  early  development  of  sensorimotor 
control. 

Though  the  experiments  with  prism  goggles  may  superficially  appear 
rather  strange  and  even  bizane,  they  are  considered  quite  relevant  to  a 
normally  occurring  process.  During  growth,  Di.  Held  explains,  head  size 
increases  until  its  linear  dimensions  become  at  least  one  and  a  half  times  as 
large  as  at  birth.  As  that  happens,  the  distance  between  the  eyes  grows, 
the  distance  between  the  ears  grows,  and  other  parts  of  the  sensory  system 
change.  Because  of  these  anatomical  changes,  the  adult  and  the  young  child 
will  have  different  sensory  inputs  for  the  same  external  conditions.  The 
fact  that  the  adult  can  handle  the  same  sensorimotor  tasks  as  the  child 
indicates  that  some  adjustment  must  have  taken  place.  Further,  the 
optical  properties  of  the  eyes  change,  too,  in  the  course  of  a  lifetime, 
and  these  changes  may  require  compensation.  "In  our  experiments  the 
changes  are  abrupt,"  says  the  investigator,  "while  in  the  natural  course  of 
things  they  are  very  slow,  but  we  think  that  some  of  the  factois  affecting 
adaptation  are  probably  the  same." 

In  the  work  with  babies,  done  under  the  immediate  supervision  of  a 
psychologist  named  Burton  White  who  is  particularly  interested  in  child 
development,  the  researchers  studied  several  groups  of  infants  who  had 
been  bom  and  were  being  reared  in  an  institution.  A  control  group  got  the 
institution's  usual  care,  which  was  adequate  for  general  health  but  pro- 
vided a  minimum  of  handling  and  other  stimulation.  Other  babies  were 
given  20  minutes'  extra  handling  every  day  for  a  month,  beginning  when 
they  were  6  days  old.  Up  to  their  fifth  month,  at  least,  these  handled 
babies  were  significantly  more  visually  attentive-shifted  their  gaze  more 
often  from  one  thing  to  another— than  the  controls.  But  there  was  no 
other  apparent  change. 

Another  group  of  babies  who  had  received  the  extra  handling  were  then 
given  a  better-than-usual  opportunity  to  move  in  their  cribs  and  to  look  at 
things  more  interesting  than  the  monotone  ceiling  and  the  white  sheets 
and  crib  liners.  The  mattresses  were  flattened  so  that  the  babies,  instead  of 
lying  in  a  trough  all  day,  could  roll  a  little  and  more  readily  move  head, 
arms,  and  trunk.  Three  times  a  day,  for  15  minutes  at  a  time,  the  babies 
were  placed  on  their  stomachs  and  the  crib  liners  removed,  thus  encourag- 
ing the  children  to  raise  their  heads  and  make  other  movements  In  order 
to  watch  the  ward  activities.  Interesting  objects  were  hung  where  the 
babies  could  see  and  swipe  at  them. 

496 


One  big  change  occurred.  At  the  median  age  of  3  months,  these  chil- 
dren began  to  reach  for  and  grasp  objects  effectively;  the  control  group 
needed  5  months.  The  results  are  what  would  have  been  predicted,  the 
investigators  point  out,  from  the  theory  that  the  development  of  sen- 
sorimotor  coordination  depends  to  a  great  extent  upon  self-initiated 
movement  and  the  accompanying  feedback  to  the  senses.  At  least  as  far  as 
the  ability  to  reach  and  grasp  is  concerned,  a  baby  apparently  develops 
faster  when  he  has  an  opportunity  to  move  and  visually  interesting  sur- 
loundings  to  encourage  him  to  move. 

Experiments  by  Dr.  Held  and  Dr.  Alan  Hein  with  cats  and  by  Dr.  Held 
and  Joseph  A.  Bauer,  Jr.,  with  monkeys  tend  to  support  the  same  theory. 
Kittens  fitted  in  infancy  with  ruffs  that  prevented  them  from  seeing  their 
paws  were  unable  a  few  weeks  later,  when  the  ruffs  had  been  removed,  to 
guide  their  paws  on  the  basis  of  vision.  Time  after  time  these  kittens 
would  pounce  on  a  slowly  swinging  ball  and  miss  it.  Much  the  same  thing 
happened  with  monkeys  raised  for  some  weeks  in  a  "holder  chair"  so 
constructed  that  they  could  use  their  hands  but  could  not  see  them.  When 
they  were  finally  allowed  to  see  one  hand,  they  could  hardly  keep  their 
eyes  from  it,  and  the  experimenters  had  difficulty  enticing  them  to  reach 
for  food  or  toys.  When  the  animals  did  reach,  they  usually  missed.  Only 
when  they  had  seen  the  hand  a  long  time,  from  10  to  20  hours,  did  they 
bring  the  movements  under  control.  Allowed  then  to  see  the  other  hand, 
the  monkeys  were  just  as  surprised— and  just  as  unable  to  control  its 
movements  to  get  what  they  wanted. 

Dr.  Held  points  out  that  persons  who  have  been  blind  since  birth  do 
achieve  sensorimotor  coordination.  In  this  case  the  sensory  feedback  that 
accompanies  movement  is  delivered  by  the  senses  of  touch  and  hearing. 
Every  once  in  a  while  a  person  long  blind  recovers  his  sight  and  then  finds 
considerable  difficulty,  for  a  time,  in  coordinating  what  he  sees  with  what 
he  does.  He  reaches  for  a  cup  of  coffee  and,  because  he  has  had  no 
opportunity  to  maintain  motor-visual  coordination,  upsets  it. 

Besides  increasing  our  basic  knowledge  of  how  the  central  nervous 
system  operates,  the  MIT  research  may  have  results  of  immediately  prac- 
tical importance.  The  work  with  babies  has  already  provided  scientific 
evidence,  the  investigators  point  out,  that  certain  kinds  of  experience  very 
early  in  life  lead  to  increased  alertness  and  faster  visual-motor  develop- 
ment. And  authorities  on  child  development  believe  that  the  growth  of 
sensorimotor  capacities  leads  to  the  growth  of  intellectual  capacity.  The 
findings  and  prospective  findings  may  also  throw  some  light  on  the  devel- 
opment of  certain  visual  disturbances,  notably  strabismus  and  amblyopia. 
The  research  may  have  a  suggestion,  too,  for  rehabilitation  workers. 
Patients  long  bedridden  lose  something  of  their  ability  to  control  the 
movements  of  their  arms  and  legs.  This  is  less  a  matter  of  muscular  weak- 
ness, Dr.  Held  suspects,  than  a  failure  to  have  performed  those  movements 
and  thus  to  have  obtained  the  feedback  from  them-conditions  that  seem 
to  be  important  to  the  maintenance  of  control. 

Voluntary  movement  and  the  accompanying  sensory  feedback  are  im- 
portant, the  investigator  sums  up, 

497 


—in  helping  infants  develop  sensonmotor  coordination; 

-in  helping  the  central  nervous  system  adjust  to  the  growth  of  the 

body; and 
-in  maintaining  normal  coordination  even  in  a  situation,  such  as  may 

occur  in  space,  where  the  incoming  signals  are  distorted. 


Compensating  for  Displaced  Sensory  Signals 

In  one  of  the  team's  early  experiments,  subjects  sat  at  a  table  ant 
looked  down  at  a  mirror,  which  earned  the  reflection  of  a  diagram.  On  tlu 
table  top,  some  inches  below  the  mirror,  lay  a  sheet  of  paper.  The  subject; 
were  asked  to  take  a  pencil  and  to  mark  with  dots  on  the  paper,  whicr 
they  could  not  see,  the  positions  of  the  four  corners  of  the  diagram.  Thei 
they  put  on  prism  goggles  for  a  while  and,  with  the  mirror  removed 
looked  at  theii  right  hands.  One  group  kept  the  hand  motionless;  anothei 
moved  it  back  and  forth;  a  third  group  had  it  moved  back  and  forth  b) 
the  investigator.  Then  the  prisms  were  removed  and  the  mirror  replaced 
and  the  subjects  were  asked  again  to  mark  the  apparent  positions  of  tin 
corners  of  the  diagram.  When  people  compensate  for  the  distortion  pio 
duced  by  prisms,  the  effect  lasts  for  a  while  after  the  prisms  are  removed 
In  this  experiment,  consequently,  the  subjects  who  had  become  adapte< 
to  the  prisms  should  have  marked  the  comers  several  inches  to  one  side  o 
the  positions  originally  marked.  The  only  subjects  who  did  mark  them  thi 
way  were  those  who  had  moved  their  hands  while  wearing  the  goggles. 

The  investigators  then  tried  the  effect  of  movement  involving  the  entirt 
body,  not  just  the  hands  and  arms.  In  a  dark  room  a  subject  was  asked  tc 
turn  his  chair  until  he  was  face  to  face  with  a  target -a  slit  of  light.  Thii 
showed  his  ability  to  judge  the  direction  of  a  target  in  reference  to  himsel 
alone.  Then  he  put  on  prism  goggles,  went  for  a  walk  of  an  hour  or  more 
came  back,  took  off  the  goggles,  and  was  tested  again.  Invariably  he  now  sav 
the  target  as  being  some  distance  to  the  side  of  where  it  actually  was.  Thi 
was  evidence  that  while  wearing  the  goggles  he  had  compensated  for  thci 
distorting  effect.  But  prism-wearing  subjects  who  had  taken  a  ride  over  tin 
same  path  in  a  wheelchair,  pushed  by  someone  else,  showed  little  or  IK 
adaptation. 

Experiments  in  directional  hearing  brought  analogous  results.  Normal]; 
a  person  locates  the  source  of  a  sound  because  the  waves  reach  one  eai 
the  closer  one,  a  split  second  before  they  reach  the  other.  But  in  this  worl 
the  subjects,  wearing  microphones,  were  exposed  to  noise  from  separate) 
sources  in  a  fashion  that  produced  rapidly  changing  dichotic  time  diffei 
ences;  that  is,  differences  in  time  of  arrival  at  the  two  ears  of  correspond 
ing  acoustic  signals.  In  effect,  the  apparent  source  of  the  sound  signals- 
like  that  of  the  visual  signals  in  persons  wearing  prisms-had  been  dii 
placed.  Subjects  who  wore  the  microphones  while  walking  up  and  down 
busy  corridor  later  showed  temporary  inaccuracies  in  locating  the  sourc 

498 


of  a  sound.  While  weaiing  the  microphones,  apparently,  they  had  compen- 
sated for  the  aural  distortion.  The  subjects  who  had  not  moved  about 
showed  no  evidence  of  such  an  effect. 

These  and  other  experiments  seemed  to  support  Dr.  Held's  idea  that 
adaptation  to  sensory  distortion  strongly  depends  upon  the  close  correla- 
tion between  signals  from  the  motor  nervous  system,  producing  the  physi- 
cal movement,  and  the  consequent  feedback  signals  to  the  sensory  system, 
showing  the  results  of  the  movement.  Seeking  further  evidence,  he  set  up 
an  experiment  in  which  the  correlation  was  impaired.  The  apparatus  was  a 
rotating  prism  arrangement  that  made  a  peison's  hand  appear  to  be  mov- 
ing back  and  forth  even  when  it  was  held  motionless.  When  the  person 
moved  his  hand,  therefore,  his  sensory  nervous  system  presumably  could 
not  distinguish  between  the  signals  resulting  from  the  actual  movement 
and  those  resulting  from  the  apparent  movement.  This  hypothesis  seemed 
to  be  borne  out  by  a  test  of  the  subjects'  accuracy  in  reaching  for  an 
object  after  they  had  been  looking  through  the  rotating  prisms.  Subjects 
who  had  not  moved  their  hands  (but  had  had  them  moved  by  the  experi- 
menter) showed  no  decline  in  their  accustomed  accuracy.  But  those  who 
had  been  moving  their  hands  themselves  showed  much  less  accuracy  than 
usual:  their  eye-hand  coordination  had  been  temporarily  impaired,  pre- 
sumably because  the  brain  had  been  getting  more  signals  than  it  could 
manage.  In  engineeiing  terms,  there  had  been  "noise"  in  the  feedback 
system. 

The  investigators  have  produced  the  same  effect  by  introducing  what 
Dr.  Held  calls  "inertial  noise."  In  one  experiment,  for  instance,  they  have 
had  a  subject  push  with  his  arm  against  an  apparatus  that,  in  spite  of  his 
efforts,  moves  his  arm  in  a  direction  opposite  to  the  one  he  wants  it  to 
take.  The  normal  relationship  between  signals  to  the  muscles  and  the 
consequent  movement  of  the  arm,  with  the  accompanying  feedback  to  the 
brain,  is  upset.  When  wearing  prisms  in  this  situation,  he  compensates 
significantly  less,  or  not  at  all. 

Further,  a  recent  experiment  shows,  the  compensatory  process  in  com- 
pletely blocked  when  there  is  a  delay  between  the  time  a  subject  moves 
Ills  hand  and  the  time  he  sees  the  result  of  this  movement.  Ordinarily,  the 
results  of  movement  reach  the  retina  with  the  speed  of  light.  To  introduce 
a  time  delay,  the  investigators  built  a  device  enabling  a  subject  to  sit  at  a 
table  and  move  a  control  slick  hidden  from  his  sight  under  a  mirror  on  the 
table.  Movement  of  the  control  stick  actuated  potentiometers  and  these  in 
turn  modulated  voltages.  When  these  voltages  were  fed  to  an  oscilloscope 
they  produced  movements  of  the  scope  trace-a  narrow  bar  l!/2  inches 
long-just  like  those  being  made  by  the  hand.  The  trace  was  projected 
onto  a  ground  glass  screen,  which  the  subject  viewed  in  the  mirror  over  his 
hand.  In  sum,  though  the  subject  could  not  see  his  hand,  he  could  watch 
every  movement  it  made.  When  the  voltages  were  fed  directly  to  the 
oscilloscope,  the  image  of  the  movement  was  perceived  instantaneously. 
In  this  situation,  before-and-after  tests  showed  that  a  person  wearing 
prism  goggles  adapted  himself  to  the  displacement  caused  by  the  goggles. 
But  the  voltages  could  also  be  fed  to  magnetic  tape  and  then  given  to  the 

499 


oscilloscope  after  a  delay.  In  this  case  the  visual  feedback  was  delayed;  the 
subject  did  not  see  the  movement  until  after  it  had  occurred.  The  investi- 
gators found  that  even  delays  as  small  as  270  milliseconds  completely 
eliminated  adaptation. 

Though  efforts  to  produce  and  study  the  effect  of  still  smaller  delays 
are  being  made,  Dr.  Held  is  fairly  well  satisfied  that  unless  motor-sensory 
feedback  is  virtually  simultaneous  with  movement,  the  brain  cannot 
handle  it.  Consequently,  the  nervous  system  will  not  adapt  itself  to  the 
apparent  displacement,  caused  by  prisms,  of  visual  signals.  This  is  quite  as 
it  should  be,  the  investigator  notes.  If  the  feedback  signals  did  not  have  to 
be  closely  coupled  with  the  output  signals,  the  system  would  sometimes 
be  in  chaos  trying  to  decide  which  of  several  moving  hands  was  its  own. 

Dr.  Held's  work  is  related  to  other  ongoing  research  at  MIT  under  the 
direction  of  Dr.  Hans-Lukas  Teuber,  chairman  of  the  department  of 
psychology,  dealing  with  the  "corollary  discharge"  and  the  supposed  role 
of  the  brain's  frontal  lobes  in  controlling  it.1  The  corollary  discharge 
theory  holds  that  when  the  brain  calls  for  movement,  some  of  the  signals 
go  not  to  the  muscles  concerned  but  to  the  appropriate  sensory  systems. 
In  this  way  the  senses  are  prepared  for  the  results  of  the  movement,  For 
example,  if  a  person  turns  his  head  while  looking  at  an  object,  the  image 
of  this  object  moves  across  the  retina  of  his  eye  just  as  it  would  if  the 
object  itself  were  moving.  But  he  perceives  that  his  eyes  are  moving  and 
not  the  object;  the  retinal  change  has  been  discounted.  And  the  discount- 
ing occurs,  Dr.  Held  postulates,  because  the  sensory  feedback  (occurring 
as  the  result  of  movement)  is  matched  to  the  corollary  discharge  (occur- 
ring as  the  result  of  the  order  to  move).  When  the  feedback  is  not  matched 
to  the  discharge,  the  movement  is  perceived  in  the  world  outside. 

Several  investigators  have  reported  recently  that  adaptation,  as  mea- 
sured by  the  accuracy  with  which  hand  and  arm  reach  toward  a  target 
after  prisms  have  been  worn,  may  result  from  a  change  in  the  felt  position 
of  the  parts  of  the  body.  Under  this  view,  a  person  misjudges  the  location 
of  the  target  because  he  no  longer  accurately  feels  the  position  of  his 
reaching  arm.  But  after  a  series  of  experiments  involving,  among  other 
things,  the  ability  of  subjects  to  locate  with  the  unseen  right  hand  various 
positions  taken  by  the  left  hand,  out  of  sight  under  the  desk  top,  the  MIT 
group  offers  another  view. 

Before  wearing  prisms,  the  subjects,  blindfolded,  could  mark  the  posi- 
tions of  the  left  hand  fairly  accurately;  after  wearing  prisms,  they  made 
mistakes  of  the  kind  to  be  expected -marking  locations  to  the  right  of  tlie 
actual  location,  for  example,  if  the  prisms  had  made  objects  seem  dis- 
placed to  the  left.  The  investigators  ask  why  a  person  should  misreach  for 
his  hand  following  prism  adaptation  when  he  doesn't  need  vision  to  reach 
for  it  accurately -when  he  can  feel  his  way  to  it,  so  to  speak. 

Dr.  Held  offers  the  following  hypothesis:  In  this  case,  there  are  actually 
two  different  but  interacting  modes  of  reaching.  One  involves  the  felt 


'"Exploring  the  Brain's  Functions."  In  Research  Project  Summaries  No,  2,  Na- 
tional Institute  of  Mental  Health:  Public  Health  Service  Publication  No.  1208-2,  1965. 


500 


positions  of  the  two  arms,  but  there  is  no  reason  for  either  of  the  two  felt 
positions  to  be  changed  by  adaptation,  and  they  probably  do  not  change. 
The  change  that  occurs  lies  in  the  other  mode  of  reaching,  which  is  based 
upon  the  matching  of  arm  movements  with  the  orientation  of  the  head.  It 
is  reasonable  to  suppose  that  adaptation  changes  the  direction  of  reaching 
with  respect  to  the  orientation  of  head  to  target.  But  in  the  case  consid- 
ered, perhaps  the  effect  of  the  change  is  modified  by  the  influence  of  the 
other  mode  of  reaching.  Hence  a  person  who  has  just  taken  off  prisms 
should  make  smaller  mistakes  in  reaching  for  his  unseen  hand  than  in 
reaching  for  a  visible  target.  The  experimental  evidence  seems  to  point  in 
this  direction. 


The  Development  of  Visual  Control 

A  few  years  ago  a  research  group  at  the  University  of  California,  River- 
side, found  that  when  kittens  were  restrained  from  walking,  they  devel- 
oped marked  deficiencies  in  the  visual  control  of  activity.  Why?  Probably, 
thought  the  California  group,  because  the  kittens  had  lacked  a  variety  of 
visual  stimulation.  More  likely,  thought  the  MIT  researchers,  because  they 
had  lacked  visual  stimulation  correlated  with  movement. 

To  get  additional  facts,  Dr.  Held  and  Dr.  Alan  Hein  ran  an  experiment 
that  has  been  much  admired  for  its  ingenuity.  The  apparatus  was  a  sort  of 
merry-go-round,  with  a  center  post  supporting  a  rotating  beam.  Attached 
to  one  arm  of  the  beam  was  a  kitten,  which  was  able  to  walk  around  the 
post,  to  jump  up,  and  to  turn  toward  the  right  and  the  left.  Attached  to 
the  other  arm  was  a  little  box  carrying  another  kitten.  Any  movements 
made  by  the  first  kitten  were  transmitted  to  the  box,  so  that  the  second 
kitten  made  them,  too,  but  passively  instead  of  on  its  own.  The  center 
post  and  the  surrounding  circular  wall  of  the  apparatus  displayed  a  pattern 
of  stripes,  so  each  kitten  received  essentially  the  same  visual  stimulation. 

The  pairs  of  kittens  were  raised  in  darkenss  till  they  were  1 0  weeks  old; 
then  they  were  brought  out  3  hours  a  day  for  experience  in  the  apparatus. 
Ten  days  later  the  active  kittens  responded  normally  to  several  tests  of 
visual  responses.  They  put  out  their  forepaws  to  ward  off  collision  when 
gently  lowered  toward  a  surface;  blinked  at  an  approaching  object;  and, 
when  walking  on  a  sheet  of  glass  beneath  which  appeared  a  shallow  drop 
to  one  side  and  a  steep  drop  to  the  other,  avoided  the  steep  one.  The 
kittens  that  had  been  passively  moved  did  not  show  these  responses. 

To  learn  more  precisely  the  conditions  influencing  visual-motor  coordi- 
nation, Drs.  Hein  and  Held  have  continued  their  studies  .of  kittens 
(financed  largely  by  the  National  Science  Foundation  but  part  of  the  same 
general  research  program  supported  by  NIMH)  and  recently  made  a  puz- 
zling discovery.  Kittens  were  put  through  the  merry-go-round  procedure 
under  both  active  and  passive  conditions  of  movement.  But  when  a  kitten 
moved  itself,  only  its  right  eye  could  see,  the  other  being  covered  by  an 
opaque  contact  lens,  and  when  the  kitten  was  passively  moved,  only  its 
left  eye  could  see.  The  result  was  an  animal  with  one  "active"  and  one 

501 


"passive"  eye.  And  the  investigators  found  that  when  the  active  eye  was 
open,  the  kitten  could  perform  normally  in  the  tests  of  visually  controlled 
behavior;  when  the  passive  eye  was  open,  it  could  not.  There  was  no 
transfer  of  information;  one  eye  was  disassociated  from  the  other,  as  if 
this  had  been  a  split-brain  study  and  the  connections  had  actually  been 
severed. 

Additional  work  supports  this  finding.  Kittens  were  raised  so  that  they 
could  see  their  forepaws  with  one  eye  but  not  the  other.  This  second  eye, 
though,  could  see  just  about  everything  else,  and  the  animals  were  free  to 
move.  Then  the  experimenters  tested  the  kittens'  ability,  first  with  one 
eye  open  and  then  the  other,  to  reach  for  and  hit  an  object.  This  ability 
was  found  to  be  present  only  when  the  eye  that  had  always  been  able  to 
watch  the  forepaws  was  open. 

"Here  is  a  failure  of  information  to  pass  with  an  intact  biain,"  the 
investigators  comment.  "It  is  very  surprising."  As  yet  they  have  no  ex- 
planation, but  they  point  out  that  the  findings  are  consistent  with  the 
central  idea  guiding  the  group's  research-that  movement  and  feedback  are 
essential  to  the  development  of  eye-hand  coordination  in  infants  and  an 
important  source  of  the  information  enabling  an  adult  to  correct  for 
sensory  distortion. 

In  work  with  animals,  the  investigators  are  now  trying  to  study  what 
actually  happens  in  the  brain  under  conditions  like  those  used  in  the 
experiments.  This  phase  of  the  research  may  or  may  not  clear  up  the 
puzzle  of  the  disassociated  eyes,  but  at  least,  Dr.  Held  believes,  it  should 
help  identify  those  parts  of  the  brain  essential  to  the  development  and 
maintenance  of  the  ability  to  direct  a  visually  guided  reach  and  grasp.  This 
is  one  of  the  primary  abilities  enabling  man  to  orient  himself  to,  and  get 
along  in,  the  world  around  him. 

Research  Grant:  MH  7642 
Date  of  Interview;  August,  1966 

References: 

Efstathiou,  Aglaia,  Bauer,  J.,  Greene,  Martha,  and  Held,  R.  Altered  reaching  following 
adaptation  to  optical  displacement  of  the  hand.  /.  Exp.  Psychol.,  1966,  in  press. 

Held,  R.  Plasticity  in  sensory-motor  systems.  Sclent.  American,  1965,  21  3,  5. 

Held,  R.  Plasticity  in  sensorimotor  coordination.  Paper  for  XVIII  International  Con- 
gress of  Psychology,  1966. 

Held,  R  ,  Efstathiou,  Aglaia,  and  Greene,  Martha.  Adaptation  to  displaced  and  delayed 
visual  feedback  from  the  hand./.  Exp.  Psychol.,  1966,72,  6,  887-891. 

Held,  R.,  and  Freedman,  S.  S.  Plasticity  in  human  sensorimolor  control.  Science, 
1963,142,3591. 

Held,  R.,  and  Hein,  A.  Movement-produced  stimulation  in  the  development  of  visually 
guided  behavior./  Comp.  Physiol.  Psychol.,  1963,  56,  5. 

White,  B.  L.,  and  Held,  R.  Plasticity  of  sensorimotor  development  in  the  human  infant. 
In  Judy  F.  Rosenblith  and  W.  Allinsmith  (Eds.),  The  causes  of  behavior:  readings  in 
child  development  and  educational  psychology.  Boston:  Allyn  &  Bacon,  1966, 


502 


Investigator. 

Benson  E.  Ginsburg,  Ph.D. 

University  of  Chicago 

Chicago,  III. 

Prepared  by 

Herbert  Yahraes 

A  person's  experiences  during  infancy  and  childhood  work  powei  fully 
to  shape  his  later  attitudes  and  personality.  Indeed,  according  to  psycho- 
analytic theory,  they  largely  determine  the  state  of  his  mental  health. 
Looking  for  scientific  proof  of  this  concept,  a  number  of  investigatois 
have  reported  evidence  linking  mental  illness  with  stressful  situations  in 
early  life.  Animal  studies,  too,  have  stressed  the  importance  of  early  ex- 
perience. Now  comes  a  geneticist  asking  that  research  into  the  causes  of 
behavior  consider  more  rigorously  the  effects  of  heredity.  Early  experi- 
ences may  indeed  affect  later  behavior  tremendously,  reports  Benson  E. 
Ginsburg,  professor  of  biology  at  the  University  of  Chicago.  Or,  depending 
upon  the  genetic  situation,  they  may  not  affect  it  at  all. 

As  part  of  his  evidence,  this  investigator  and  his  students  subjected 
carefully  bred  mice  to  certain  types  of  stress  during  infancy.  Some  of 
them,  as  adults,  became  significantly  more  aggressive  than  usual;  others 
became  less  aggressive;  others  showed  no  effect.  The  results  depended 
upon  the  strains  to  which  the  mice  belonged:  that  is,  upon  heredity.  In 
most  cases  the  results  depended  also  upon  the  time  when  the  stress  had 
been  encoimtered-during  all  the  first  4  weeks  of  life,  or  only  the  first  2, 
or  only  the  second  2.  "All  mice,"  Dr.  Ginsbuig  notes,  "are  not  created 
equal.'1 

Because  work  by  himself  and  other  researchers  shows  a  strong  relation- 
ship between  an  animal's  inheritance  and  certain  facets  of  its  behavior, 
including  some  abnormal  behavior  under  stress,  the  investigator  believes 
the  same  relationship  may  well  hold  true  in  people.  If  one  can  select  for 
aggressive  strains  in  rabbits,  guinea  pigs,  dogs,  fighting  cocks,  and  bulls, 
and  if  equivalent  early  experience  can  have  dlffeiential  effects  in  mice, 
depending  on  the  genetic  background,  he  asks,  why  should  it  be  supposed 
that  human  beings  all  respond  to  a  given  situation  in  the  same  way, 
regardless  of  their  inheritance?  He  agrees  with  psychotherapists  that 
psychological  factors  are  important,  but  he  holds  that  these  must  act  on  n 
biological  substratum— on  inherited  tendencies  and  mechanisms. 

In  recent  work  with  mice,  Dr.  Ginsburg  believes  that  his  research  group 
has  uncovered  the  inherited  mechanism  responsible  for  a  certain  kind  of 
abnormal  behavior.  Apparently  the  mechanism  lies  in  a  particular  cell 

503 


layer  in  one  structure  of  the  brain,  and  appaiently  its  manifestations  can 
be  treated  chemically  in  ceitain  cases.  In  some  other  mice,  the  same  kind 
of  behavior  seems  to  have  a  different  cause  and  is  not  affected  by  the 
same  chemotherapy. 

Dr.  Ginsburg's  work  is  supported  by  a  grant  from  the  National  Institute 
of  Mental  Health.  His  main  laboratory  animals  are  highly  inbred  mice 
(every  individual  in  a  strain  essentially  an  identical  twin  of  every  other 
individual),  wolves,  coyotes,  and  dogs. 


Stress  Mechanism  in  Mice 

The  investigator  and  his  group  have  set  out  to  study  differences  in 
behavior  among  closely  related  animals,  to  learn  what  laws  of  genetics  are 
followed  in  the  inheritance  of  these  differences,  and  to  determine  what 
body  or  brain  tissues  are  affected  by  the  differences  in  heredity  and, 
therefore,  give  rise  to  the  differences  in  behavior. 

In  mice  the  principal  characteristic  under  study  is  susceptibility  to 
audiogenic  seizures-epilepticlike  convulsions  induced  by  the  ringing  of  a 
bell.  The  grantee  chose  this  characteristic  because;  (a)  it  leads  to  behavior 
of  an  extreme  kind,  relatively  easy,  therefore,  to  identify;  (£)  it  is,  like 
some  types  of  abnormal  behavior  in  human  beings,  triggered  by  stress— in 
this  case,  sound;  (f)  it  appears  only  in  certain  strains  of  mice  and  conse- 
quently must  be  under  genetic  control— a  deduction  Dr.  Ginsburg  and  an 
associate,  Dr.  Starbuck  Miller,  have  demonstrated,  through  breeding  ex- 
periments, to  be  correct. 

Some  highlights  of  the  findings  to  date: 

1 .  Three  strains  of  mice  highly  susceptible  to  seizures  show  a  character- 
istic difference  in  one  tiny  area  of  the  brain.  It  appears  to  be  caused 
by  a  difference  in  either  the  amount  or  the  activity  of  one  or  more 
closely  related  enzymes. 

The  difference  came  to  light  in  the  course  of  a  meticulous,  area-by-area 
study  of  activity  involving  adenosine  triphosphate,  or  ATP.  This  is  the 
compound  supplying  most  of  the  energy  for  biochemical  reactions,  such 
as  those  concerned  with  moving  the  muscles,  building  proteins,  and  flash- 
ing nerve  signals.  The  energy  is  made  available  when  ATP  is  broken  down 
in  an  almost  instantaneous  reaction  involving  the  enzyme  ATP-ase.  The 
investigation  centered  on  this  reaction  because  of  an  earlier  report,  which 
has  not  yet  been  verified,  that  the  brains  of  seizure-prone  mice  contained 
less  of  the  enzyme  than  the  brains  of  other  mice. 

The  Ginsburg  group  incubated  slices  of  brain  tissue  in  a  medium  con- 
taining ATP  or  a  related  compound  and  then  examined  them  under  the 
microscope  for  a  brown  stain  produced  by  one  of  the  chemicals  resulting 
from  the  breakdown  of  that  compound,  The  darker  the  stain,  the  greater 
the  amount  of  ATP  that  had  been  broken  down  and,  presumably,  the 
greater  the  amount  of  ATP-ase  (or  the  greater  its  activity)  in  the  area  of 
the  brain  under  examination, 

504 


Of  all  the  areas  studied,  a  difference  between  seizure-prone  and  seizure- 
resistant  mice  was  found  in  only  one-a  thin  layer  of  cells  in  the  hippo- 
campus (specifically,  the  granular  cell  layer  of  the  dentate  fascia),  which  is 
one  of  the  oldest  parts  of  the  cortex.  In  the  mice  susceptible  to  seizures, 
this  layer  stained  consistently  darker  than  in  the  other  mice.  It  also 
stained  darker  when  triphosphates  other  than  ATP  were  used  in  the  incu- 
bating medium,  suggesting  that  the  activity  may  be  caused  by  other 
enzymes  that  break  down  this  class  of  compounds. 

Breeding  and  other  experiments  showed  that  the  staining  differences 
were  inborn,  with  two  pairs  of  genes  apparently  being  involved,  and  did 
not  arise  from  the  seizures  themselves.  The  experiments  seemed  to  show 
also  that  whatever  led  to  the  staining  difference  led  as  well  to  the  seizuies, 
a  finding  that  was  given  additional  support  when  structural  diffeiences 
were  discovered  in  the  cell  layer. 

In  sum,  the  research  group  reports,  these  findings  indicate  that  the 
epilepticlike  seizures  of  some  strains  of  mice  are  indeed  traceable  to  an 
inherited  difference  most  probably  involving  one  small  part  of  the  brain. 
In  the  test  tube,  at  least,  this  difference  apparently  involves  a  reaction 
related  to  energy  release.  It  is  conjectured  that  in  the  brains  of  these  mice, 
a  certain  kind  of  stress  leads  to  abnormal  energy  release  due  to  a  genetic 
deficiency  in  a  localized  biochemical  mechanism. 

Whether  or  not  these  findings  relating  to  audiogenic  seizures  in  mice 
can  be  applied  to  epilepsy  in  human  beings  remains  to  be  determined. 
Until  recently,  Dr.  Ginsburg  notes,  little  was  known  of  the  relationship 
between  seizures  in  mice  and  those  in  people.  But  electroencephalographic 
studies  by  other  investigators  now  indicate  that  the  seizures  induced  in 
mice  are  quite  similar  to  those  found  in  some  forms  of  epilepsy. 

2.  The  biochemical  mechanisms  involved  in  the  type  of  abnormal  be- 
havior under  study  differ  from  animal  to  animal,  depending  upon 
what  genes  have  been  inherited 

In  one  strain  of  seizure-prone  mice,  the  staining  reaction,  instead  of 
being  typical,  resembles  that  found  in  mice  resistant  to  seizures.  (This 
strain  arose,  by  natural  mutation,  from  a  seizure-resistant  strain,)  Even 
strains  showing  the  typical  ATP-ase  reaction  differ  in  other  characteristics 
—for  example,  the  age  at  which  they  arc  most  susceptible  to  seizures  and 
the  effect  of  repeated  exposure  to  noise,  which  in  some  strains  increases 
susceptibility. 

"A  behavior  peculiarity,"  Dr.  Ginsburg  comments,  "can  be  a  kind  of 
stew  with  a  lot  of  ingredients  in  it."  As  further  evidence  he  cites  experi- 
ments to  learn  whether  or  not  seizure-prone  mice  could  be  chemically 
protected.  Some  of  them  could  be.  The  chemical  used  was  monosoclium 
glutamate,  which  is  closely  related  to  glutamic  acid,  a  compound  that 
appears  in  the  brain  and  has  been  widely  investigated  because  of  reported 
findings  that  it  can  improve  learning  ability.  Following  injections  of 
rnonosodium  glutamate  (which  also  has  a  common  use,  to  accent  the 
flavor  of  food),  mice  of  certain  strains  had  fewer  and  lighter  seizures.  Mice 
of  other  seizure-prone  strains,  however,  were  not  affected. 

505 


3.  Mice  that  have  seizures  when  exposed  to  a  ringing  bell  also  respond 
abnormally  to  a  stressful  learning  situation. 

An  ordinary  mouse  that  has  learned  to  run  through  simple  mazes  and 
then  is  confronted  with  a  maze  a  little  too  complex  for  his  learning  ability 
will  keep  on  trying  foi  a  while.  But  a  seizure-pione  mouse  goes  to  pieces, 
as  evidenced  by  squeaking  and  by  frantic  jumping.  An  injection,  before- 
hand, of  monosodium  glutamate  will  lead  to  normal  behavior,  but  only  if 
the  mouse  is  from  a  stiain  in  which  the  compound  will  reduce  seizures. 

4.  Mice  become  less  aggressive  if  they  have  had  a  seizure  while  young. 
Along  with  the  other  findings,  this  is  believed  to  suggest  a  connection 
between  the  seizure  mechanistn  and  various  forms  of  behavior  under 
stress, 

In  some  human  beings  with  mental  disorders,  therapy  involving  convul- 
sive seizures-induced  by  shock  treatment-is  followed  by  a  return  to 
normal  behavior.  Dr.  Ginsburg  wondered  whether  or  not  seizures  would 
affect  the  later  behavior  of  mice.  As  the  trait  to  be  studied,  he  and  his 
co-workeis  chose  aggressiveness,  measured  by  the  proportion  of  times  two 
mice  with  the  same  backgrounds  attempt  to  fight  when  given  the  oppor- 
tunity. 

The  mice  came  from  two  strains  highly  susceptible  to  seizures,  Members 
of  both  these  strains,  as  well  as  those  of  a  third  seizure-prone  strain,  hart 
been  shown  to  be  considerably  more  aggressive  than  members  of  three 
seizure-resistant  strains. 

Seizures  were  induced  when  the  animals  were  28  days  old.  Each  mouse 
was  then  housed  alone  until  its  75th  day,  when  the  fighting  trials  started. 
Results;  In  one  strain  the  mice  that  had  had  seizures  tried  to  fight  on  only 
37  percent  of  the  occasions  as  against  72  percent  for  the  control  mice,  In 
the  other  stiain  the  figuie  was  56  percent  as  against  80. 

5 .  The  effects  of  stressful  situations  during  infancy  are  governed  at  least 
in  part  by  heredity. 

In  so-called  "handling"  experiments,  a  young  mouse  is  lifted  from  its 
home  cage  every  day  and  placed  for  a  few  minutes  by  itself  in  an  un- 
familiar place,  or  it  is  briefly  exposed  to  cold  or  pain.  When  mice  that 
have  been  subjected  to  such  stress  reach  adulthood,  their  behavior  is  com- 
pared with  that  of  mice  reared  without  the  stress.  A  common  criterion  is 
aggressiveness. 

As  noted  earlier  in  this  report,  Dr.  Ginsburg's  group  has  found  that  in 
most  cases  studied,  the  results-a  significant  increase  in  aggressiveness,  a 
significant  decrease,  or  no  change-depend  both  upon  the  strain  of  mice 
and  the  period  of  stress.  For  instance,  one  strain  becomes  more  aggressive 
if  handled  during  its  second  2  weeks  of  life  but  not  if  handled  only  during 
its  first  2.  Another  becomes  less  aggressive  if  handled  during  its  first  2 
weeks  but  not  if  handled  only  during  its  second  2, 

506 


The  grantee  compares  the  implication  of  these  findings  with  the  in- 
terpretation given  to  the  results  of  earlier  handling  experiments,  princi- 
pally with  rats.  The  older  results  had  been  taken  to  show  that  early  experi- 
ence profoundly  affected  various  kinds  of  behavior,  including  aggressive- 
ness, in  a  stereotyped -way.  Thus,  the  grantee  observes,  animal  experimen- 
tation had  seemed  to  bear  out  Freudian  theory. 

But  the  findings  of  the  older  work  were  in  most  cases  the  averages  of 
the  results  obtained  from  experimenting  with  mixed-or  average-  popula- 
tions. "When  you  break  down  a  population  into  a  number  of  compo- 
nents by  using  pure  strains,"  Dr.  Ginsburg  continues,  "you  find  that  early 
experience  in  one  genetic  situation  has  a  very  different  effect  on  later 
measures  of  behavior  from  early  experience  in  another  genetic  situation. 
One  individual  may  be  so  well  buffered  that  early  stressful  experiences 
show  no  effect  later  on.  But  they  may  decidedly  change  another  indi- 
vidual in  either  one  direction  or  the  other. 

"One  mouse  is  not  the  same  as  another  mouse,  and  certainly  one  person 
is  not  the  same  as  another  person.  This  is  true  in  respect  not  only  to  the 
experiences  an  individual  has  had  but  also  to  his  potentiality  forieacting 
to  such  experiences." 

Consequently,  in  Dr.  Ginsburg's  opinion,  the  way  drugs  and  other 
forms  of  therapy  affect  behavior  can  be  expected  to  show  considerable 
variation. 


Inherited  Aggressiveness 

A  tendency  to  engage  in  certain  kinds  of  aggressive  behavior,  the 
grantee  notes,  is  definitely  inherited.  For  example,  rabbits  of  one  partic- 
ular strain,  first  found  at  the  Jackson  Memorial  Laboratories,  Bar  Haibor, 
Maine,  will  almost  invariably  fight  an  intruder.  A  person  who  pokes  a 
hand  toward  them  or  tries  to  pick  them  up  will  be  clawed.  This  happens 
no  matter  how  the  animals  have  been  raised.  Dr.  Ginsburg  has  given  young 
ones  away  as  Easter  bunnies  to  see  what  would  happen  if  they  were 
brought  up  with  tender,  loving  care.  They  turned  out  mean. 

Terriers  have  been  selected  for  aggressiveness -and  so  successfully  that 
investigators  at  Bar  Harbor  have  found  it  almost  impossible  to  raise  a  litter 
of  four  or  more  wirehaired  fox  terrier  pups  under  natural  conditions.  If 
the  litter  is  kept  together  after  weaning  and  one  puppy  goes  down  in  a 
fight,  the  others  gang  up  on  him.  One  puppy  can  defend  itself  against  two 
of  its  littermates,  but  not  three.  Eventually  it  is  mutilated  or  killed.  On 
the  other  hand,  when  wirehaireds  are  raised  in  isolation  from  mother  and 
littermates  and  placed  together  only  after  having  been  weaned  from  the 
bottle*  no  fights  develop.  The  innate  genetic  aggressiveness  is  not  e 


circumstances,  is  bad.  By  genetic  selection  the  program  now  produces  a 
high  proportion  of  German  shepherds  that  have  only  the  desirable  trait. 

Eventually,  the  University  of  Chicago  geneticists  hope  to  learn  the 
physiological  basis  foi  aggressiveness  in  rabbits,  for  the  diffeienccs  in 
aggressiveness  of  strains  of  mice  exposed  to  stress,  and  for  differences  in 
the  aggressiveness  of  a  wild  animal,  the  wolf,  and  a  related  domestic 
animal,  the  dog, 


The  Socialization  of  Wolves 

Preliminary  work  with  wolves  has  been  underway  for  several  years.  It 
aims  at  noting  marked  behavioral  diffeiences  between  wolves  and  some  of 
their  relatives,  principally  coyotes  and  dogs,  and  then  eventually  at  learn- 
ing the  physiological  bases  for  these  differences.  The  wolves  come  from 
the  Chicago  Zoological  Park  and  are  wild,  in  the  sense  that  they  have  not 
been  handled  by  human  beings.  (They  were  first  housed  in  pens  that  had 
been  occupied  by  the  university's  watchdogs.  The  watchdogs  never 
learned  to  work  the  latches;  the  wolves  learned  easily,  and  the  first  wolf 
out  would  open  all  the  other  pens.  The  researches  had  to  install  locks.) 

So  far  the  investigatois  have  learned,  among  other  things,  that  woives 
can  be  socialized  at  any  age  and  that  tranquilizing  drugs  have  no  perm- 
anent effect  on  the  process  by  which  this  is  done.  The  first  part  of  this 
finding,  Dr.  Ginsburg  repoits,  demands  a  reinterpretation  of  the  widely 
held  hypothesis  of  a  critical  period  for  socialization-a  limited  time  early 
in  life  during  which,  and  only  during  which,  an  animal  can  learn  to  accept 
human  beings.  That  hypothesis  is  based  mainly  on  work  with  dogs  and 
birds. 

Socializing  an  adult  wolf  is  a  heroic  job.  As  the  grantee  outlines  it,  you 
begin  by  entering  the  pen.  The  wolf,  becoming  extremely  upset,  trembles, 
moves  away,  defecates,  tries  to  get  out.  You  have  to  accustom  it  to  your 
presence  by  going  to  the  pen  time  and  again  and  just  sitting  there,  This 
phase  is  known  as  the  aversive,  emotional  stage. 

As  the  animal  gets  over  its  extreme  fear,  Dr.  Ginsburg  continues,  it 
enters  the  stage  of  the  slightly  aggressive  approach.  It  takes  cautious  steps 
to  investigate  you.  Now  is  a  critical  time  because  you  want  to  establish 
contact,  yet  any  move  may  provoke  either  fear  or  aggression.  So  you 
watch  for  the  signs  that  tell  how  far  you  may  go.  Curled  lips,  raised  hair, 
growls,  and,  especially,  bared  canine  teeth  are  all  warnings  that  the  wolf  is 
in  a  fighting  humor.  In  addition  to  these  signs,  if  the  tail  droops  and  the 
ears  are  back,  you  know  that  the  animal  is  still  quite  frightened  and  that 
you  may  approach  it -very  slowly,  in  full  view,  and  without  jerky  move- 
ments. 

If  the  tail  is  held  high,  however,  and  the  ears  forward,  the  wolf  means 
to  attack.  Stand  your  ground,  and  you'll  be  snapped  at  and  nosed,  Re- 
treat, and  the  wolf  will  be  at  you  every  time  you  enter  the  pen.  Is  there  an 
odor  of  fear?  "We  don't  think  so,"  answers  Dr.  Ginsburg,  "because  we've  all 
been  as  frightened  as  we  could  be.  But  the  animal  doesn't  know  that.  How 

508 


it  reacts  to  us  is  not  a  matter  of  how  we  feel  but  of  what  we  do" 
Apparently  what  the  experiments  weais  has  a  hearing,  too  Gkms  and 
other  protective  clothing  aie  not  worn  because  in  the  beginning  they 
appear  to  frighten  the  animal  and  then  to  make  it  more  aggressive. 

Next  comes  the  investigative  stage,  duiing  which  the  animal  rubs 
against  you,  tugs  at  your  clothing  and  may  mark  you  with  urine. 

Attempts  to  domesticate  the  animal  during  the  second  or  third  phase 
will  bring  back  the  fear  response  and  retard  the  socialization  process 

Finally  you  are  accepted.  The  wolf  greets  you  by  wagging  its  tail,  and  it 
comes  up  to  be  patted  and  scratched  and  to  mouth  your  chin.  Then, 
having  accepted  you,  it  soon  comes  to  accept  other  human  beings  It  has 
been  socialized. 

Once  socialized,  the  grantee  reports,  adult  wolves  remain  friendly  with 
human  beings— for  a  considerable  time,  anyway-even  after  taking  up  life 
again  with  their  brethren.  Eighteen  months  after  a  socialized  adult  has 
been  returned  to  the  zoo,  where  it  ran  with  wild  wolves  and  was  not 
handled  by  the  keepers,  the  Ginsburg  group  took  it  back.  It  was  still  easy 
to  handle. 

In  contrast,  when  a  thoroughly  socialized  young  wolf  was  left  alone 
with  an  unsocialized  littermate  at  the  age  of  4'/i  months,  it  regressed. 
Eighteen  months  later  it  was  as  wild  as  the  unhandled  wolf  and  had  to  be 
socialized  all  over  again. 

Tranquihzers,  over  the  long  run,  do  not  speed  the  process.  Under  any 
of  three  quite  different  compounds-chlorpromazine,  reserpine, 
chlordiazepoxide-adult  wolves  can  be  socialized  in  a  matter  of  days 
rather  than  months,  but  when  the  drugs  are  withdrawn,  the  socialized 
behavior  drops  away  and  the  animal  is  back  where  it  started  from.  In 
other  words,  tranquihzers  can  help  a  wolf  overcome  fear  and  aggressive 
tendencies  but  only  during  the  period  of  tranquilization.  The  answer  to 
the  important  question,  Does  learning  carry  over  to  the  undntgged 
state?,  is-in  this  situation-wo. 

With  dogs,  the  story  appears  to  be  different.  When  tranquihzers  arc 
given  to  dogs  that  have  been  reared  ta  isolation  and  are,  in  consequence, 
extremely  frightened,  the  animals  adjust  to  the  world  of  man  muct 
rapidly  than  untranquilized  dogs,  and  they  do  not  regress  when  the 
dropped. 


hand.  They  comprise  a  ritual,  each  element  of  which  elicits  an  appro  p 
ate,  built-in  response.  This  symbolic  behavior  serves  to  establish  don 
nance  and  to  defend  or  claim  territory-usually  without  the  need  of  figli 
ing.  Other  symbolic  behavior  governs  courtship. 

The  grantee  finds  an  analogy  here  to  the  behavior  of  human  beings.  I 
points  out  that  psychoanalysis  is  largely  based  on  the  proposition  that 
mental  event,  as  in  a  dream  or  a  fantasy,  can  be  a  symbol  of  who; 
meaning  the  individual  is  not  consciously  aware.  Animals,  too,  deal  i 
symbols,  Dr.  Ginsburg  observes,  and  the  level  at  which  these  may  hav 
meaning  is  analogous  to,  if  not  identical  with,  a  level  generally  consideie 
to  exist  "only  in  our  own  psyche." 

Dogs  have  no  dependable  series  of  signals;  the  grantee  says  domestic! 
tion  has  cost  them  part  of  their  inheritance.  Until  the  experimenter  gets  ti 
know  the  individual  animal,  he  cannot  tell  whether  or  not  a  given  postur 
has  the  same  meaning  it  would  have  in  a  wolf. 

To  get  back  to  tranquilizers:  The  investigators  reported  during  tin 
early  part  of  this  work  that  chlordiazepoxide  seemed  to  eliminate  a  wolf: 
fear  and  bring  out  its  aggressiveness.  Much  the  same  effect  was  bciii( 
observed  with  human  beings,  a  psychiatrist  told  the  grantee,  Ccrlaii 
patients  taking  that  tranquilizer  tended  to  attack  people,  and  the  patieiils 
it  developed,  were  usually  those  with  records  of  assault  and  other  violence, 
Since  wolves,  too,  have  violence  in  their  background,  the  psychiatrist 
wondered  if  the  situations  might  not  be  analogous. 

Dr.  Ginsburg  doesn't  know  what  conclusions  the  psychiatrist  eventual- 
ly reached.  His  own  continued  observations,  however,  showed  that  the 
tranquilizer  was  not  releasing  a  wolf's  aggressiveness;  instead,  it  was 
greatly  shortening  all  the  stages  of  socialization,  including  the  first  one,  in 
which  the  animal  is  afraid,  and  the  second,  in  which  it  is  aggressive.  As 
noted  earlier,  the  drug's  effect  does  not  hold. 

Tranquilizers  did  have  one  lasting  effect.  Experimenters  who  thought 
they  were  working  with  tranquilized  wolves-as  sometimes  they  were  and 
sometimes  they  were  not-handled  the  animals  more  confidently  and  got 
better  results. 


Work  To  Be  Done 

The  grantee  and  his  group  will  continue  to  search  for  the  underlying 
biochemical  reasons  for  different  behavior.  One  project  aims  at  exploring 
other  genetic  pathways  (besides  the  one  that  apparently  involves  the 
breakdown  of  ATP  or  related  compounds)  involved  in  audiogenic  seizures, 
Another  hopes  to  learn  why  seizure-prone  mice  can  differ  from  one 
another  in  such  matters  as  the  age  of  greatest  susceptibility  and  the  cffecl 
of  early,  stressful  experiences  upon  later  aggressiveness.  Studies  of  the 
adrenal  gland,  the  catecholamines,  the  acetylcholine-cholinesterase  system 
in  the  hipocampal  area,  and  the  metabolism  of  phenylalanine-all  of  which 
are  known  or  believed  to  influence  brain  function -are  underway  in  one  or 
another  of  the  strains  showing  behavior  differences. 

510 


In  trying  to  establish  a  connection  between  a  difference  in  behavior 
and  a  difference  in  some  biochemical  characteristic,  Dr.  Ginsbnrg  observes, 
research  has  relied  mainly  on  statistical  methods.  The  grantee  prefers  to 
emphasize  breeding  experiments  designed  to  show  whether  or  not  the 
behavioral  difference  appears  only  in  animals  showing  the  biochemical 
difference.  He  believes  that  genetically  controlled  strains  must  be  used  if 
the  physiological  reasons  for  variant  behaviors  are  to  be  demonstrated 
clearly. 

Research  Grant.    MH  3361 

References: 

Giiisburg,  Benson  E.  Genetics  as  a  tool  in  the  study  of  behavior.  Perspectives  in 
Biology  and  Medicine,  1(4),  1958. 

Ginsburg,  Benson  E.  Causal  mechanisms  in  audiogenic  seizures,  In:  Psycho  physi- 
ologic, Neitropharmacologte,  et  Biochemie  de  la  Crise  Audiogene,  312:227-240, 
1963.  (Colloques  Internationaux  du  Centre  National  de  la  Recherche  Scientifique, 
Pans,  1963.) 

Ginsburg,  Benson  E.  Genetics  and  personality.  In:  Wepman,  Joseph  M.  and  Heine, 
Ralph  W.,  eds.  Concepts  of  personality  Chicago,  Aldine  Press,  1963,  p.  63-78. 

Ginsburg,  Benson  E.  and  Slatis,  Herman.  The  use  of  pure-bred  dogs  in  problems. 
Proceedings  of  the  A  mmal  Care  Panel,  1 2(4):  1 5 1  - 1 56,  1 962 . 

Ginsburg,  Benson  E.  and  Miller,  D.  Starbuck.  Genetic  factors  in  audiogenic  seizures. 
In:  Psychophysiologie,  Neuropharmacologie,  et  Biochemie  de  la  Crise  Audiogene. 
(Colloques  Internationaux  du  Centre  National  de  la  Recherche  Scientifique,  Paris, 
1963.) 


511 


Investigator. 

Steven  G.  Vandenberg,  Ph.  D. 
University  of  Louisville  School  of  Medicine 
Louisville,  Ky. 

prepared  by: 

Herbert  Yahraes 


Information  about  the  influence  of  heredity  on  the  behavior  of  hum, 
beings  is  being  obtained  by  an  Institute  grantee  through  a  long-time  slut 
of  twins,  one  of  the  most  comprehensive  investigations  of  the  kind  ov 
attempted. 

Coupled  with  the  findings  of  other  investigators,  the  results  of  II 
study  are  expected  to  answer  with  greater  assurance  than  is  now  possili 
such  questions  as: 

•  Just  wheie,  in  the  bioad  fields  of  intelligence  and  personality,  ilui 
heredity  have  its  principal  influence? 

•  Should  emphasis  on  family-child  relationships  as  a  major  0:111x0  < 
mental  illness  give  way  in  part  to  a  strengthened  emphasis  on  heicditur 
factors? 

•  What  hereditary  characteristics  seem  to  be  involved  in  the  ilcvelo] 
ment  of  mental  illness? 

•  In  predicting  whether  or  not  a  child  will  later  deviate  from  noun: 
behavior,  which  tests  of  his  intelligence  and  personality  and  which  nspocl 
of  his  early  life  are  most  useful? 

Tlie  grantee  is  Dr.  Steven  G.  Vandenberg,  research  associate  profcsso 
of  child  development  at  the  University  of  Louisville  and  director  o 
psychological  research  for  the  university's  Louisville  twin  study.  In  oiu 
part  of  this  project,  all  pairs  of  twins  of  the  same  sex  born  in  Louisvilk'; 
eight  hospitals  are  studied  during  infancy  and  the  preschool  years,  h 
another  part  of  the  project,  like-sexed  twins  already  in  the  public  mui 
parochial  school  systems  are  picked  up  as  they  become  high  school  fresh 
men  and,  once  a  year  until  they  leave  school,  given  a  battery  of  psycho 
logical  tests.  Under  plans  for  expanding  the  project,  information  about  [hi 
home  environment  of  these  older  twins  will  be  obtained  through  visits  iiml 
questionnaires,  and  the  twins  may  be  followed  after  they  leave  school, 

Psychologists  and  geneticists  have  turned  to  twin  studies  many  times  in 
the  past  for  light  on  the  roles  played  by  heredity  and  environment,  but 
the  current  project  promises  to  be  especially  valuable  because  (or)  il  lists 
more  advanced  statistical  techniques  to  estimate  the  strength  of  hereditary 
factors  and  to  select  tests  that  most  clearly  bring  out  hereditary  influ- 
ences; (6)  it  uses  a  relatively  large  sample  of  twins  (about  1 20  pairs  in  Ihc 
baby  study  and  250  pairs  in  the  high  school  study-numbers  that  may  he 

512 


considerably  increased  if  plans  foi  expansion  go  through),  thus  strength- 
ening the  validity  of  the  findings;  and  (c)  unlike  most  eailier  studies,  it 
uses  a  reliable  technique  for  distinguishing  between  fraternal  and  identical 
twins,  another  factor  adding  to  the  validity  of  the  results. 

Obstetricians  and  patents  often  make  mistakes  in  identifying  types  of 
twins,  but  analysis  of  blood  groups -the  technique  used  in  this  study-is 
reliable  at  least  90  percent  of  the  time.  This  means  that  in  possibly  1  pair 
of  fiaternal  twins  out  of  10,  both  twins  will  have  the  same  blood  factors 
and  will  appear  on  this  basis  to  be  identical.  Such  cases  can  virtually 
always  be  classified  correctly,  however,  by  differences  in  the  structure  of 
the  heads,  hands,  and  teeth,  in  the  shape  of  the  ears,  or  in  the  color  and 
shape  of  the  eyes.  (In  a  doubtful  case,  absolute  proof  could  be  offered 
only  by  the  skin-graft  test.) 

The  study  of  baby  twins  was  instituted  by  Frank  Falkuer,  M.D.,  now 
head  of  the  Department  of  Pediatiics  in  the  University's  School  of  Medi- 
cine, to  obtain  fuller  information  on  how  heredity  affects  physical  devel- 
opment. Dr.  Falkner  brought  in  Dr.  Vandenberg,  who  added  the  study  of 
high  school  twins.  In  addition  to  NIMH  research  grants  to  Dr.  Vandenberg 
and  his  appointment  as  a  research  career  development  investigator,  the 
work  as  a  whole  has  been  supported  by  other  NIH  and  USPHS  grants'  and 
by  a  small  National  Science  Foundation  grant.  Under  a  new  grant  from 
the  Institute  of  Child  Health  and  Human  Development,  Dr.  Vandenberg 
hopes  eventually  to  establish  a  Center  for  Human  Behavior  Genetics, 
where  additional  studies  would  be  undertaken. 

Before  going  to  Louisville  in  1961,  Dr.  Vandenberg  had  already  done 
considerable  work  on  heredity  and  behavior,  principally  as  director  of 
psychological  studies  for  the  hereditary  abilities  study  of  the  University  of 
Michigan,  which  NIMH  and  the  McGregor  Fund  of  Detroit  supported. 
That  project,  too,  studied  twins-82  pairs,  mainly  from  high  schools.  The 
grantee's  findings  discussed  in  this  report  are  based  in  part  on  preliminary 
results  from  the  Louisville  study  of  high  school  twins,  but  in  greater  part 
on  recently  published  reports  of  the  Michigan  project  and  on  new  analyses 
by  Dr.  Vandenberg  of  data  from  a  review  of  other  twin  studies. 

The  findings  may  be  summed  up  as  follows: 

1.  Heredity  influences  human  behavior  in  many  of  the  aspects  com- 
monly measured  by  psychological  tests.  Its  influence  shows  up  strongly 
even  in  some  types  of  behavior,  such  as  performance  in  arithmetic  and 
spelling,  in  which  people  have  had  a  good  deal  of  training. 

2,  Four  of  the  six  factors  found  by  the  late  Dr.  L.  L.  Thurstone  to  be 
essential  aspects  of  intelligence  and  included  in  the  widely  used  Primary 
Mental   Abilities   test   are   under  strong  genetic  control.  These  are  the 
factors  making  for  the  abilities  described  as   numerical,  verbal  (under- 
standing of  words),  spatial  (the  ability  to  perceive  patterns  and  structures 
and  to  move  them  around  in  the  mind,  keeping  the  relationships  intact), 
and  word  fluency.  Two  other  abilities  included  in  the  test,  reasoning  and 


1  RG  5527,  A  4847,  H  7233,  HD  482. 

513 


memory,  do  not  show  a  strong  hereditary  influence-quite  possibly  be- 
cause the  test  does  not  deal  with  enough  of  the  factors  comprising  them. 
Memory  for  names,  as  an  example,  is  included  in  the  test,  but  it  has  been 
found  unrelated  to  memory  for  faces,  which  is  not.  Further,  a  person's 
willingness  to  memorize  the  items  during  the  testing  session  is  highly 
influenced  by  his  attitude  towards  the  testing,  so  the  test  measures  his 
motivation  as  much  as  his  memory. 

3,  Motor  skills  decidedly  affected  by  heredity  include  dexterity  in 
using  tweezers  to  place  small  pins  into  holes,  the  ability  to  soit  cards  fast 
and  accurately,  and  the  ability  to  walk  a  beam  without  losing  balance.  But 
tests  of  hand  steadiness  and  body  sway  give  insignificant  results. 

4.  In  the  area  of  personality,  one  group  of  tests  shows  a  high  degree  of 
heritability  for  the  factors  of  activity,  vigor,  impulsiveness,  and  sociabil- 
ity—but not  for  dominance,  stability,  or  reflectiveness.  Other  tests  indicate 
a  relatively  strong  hereditary  influence  upon  neuroticism,  the  display  of 
nervous  tension,  and  control  of  the  will;  and  there  is  some  evidence  that 
the  degree  to  which  a  person  is  an  extrovert  or  an  introvert  is  partially 
determined  by  heredity.  However,  most  measures  of  personality  traits  fail 
to  disclose  any  significant  genetic  role  for  them -evidence,  Dr.  Vanden- 
berg  believes,  not  that  personality  is  little  affected  by  heredity  but  that 
present  tests  are  measuring  traits  primarily  affected  by  environment.  He 
points  out  that  such  tests  are  right  and  good  for  most  applications  but  not 
for  studies  of  hereditary  factors. 

5.  Tests  of  musical  ability  and  preference  have  been  even  less  successful 
than  those  of  personality  in  pointing  to  a  hereditary  factor.  Perhaps  only 
the  exceptional  talent  of  great  composers  and  musicians  has  such  a  factor, 
the  investigator  suggests.  Perhaps,  in  the  case  of  the  musical  and  certain 
other  abilities,   heredity  studies  might  make  greater  headway  if  they 
looked   for  innate  defects  in  basic  skills  rather  than  for  innate  profi- 
ciencies. 

6,  Some  of  the  ways  in  which  the  body  reacts  to  stressful  situations 
appear  to  be  influenced  by  heredity.  In  a  laboratory  situation,  most  of  the 
twins  in  the  Michigan  study  responded  to  mild  stress— induced  by  a  sud- 
den flash  of  light  and  the  fall  of  a  hammer-by  changes  in  (a)  the  rate  of 
the  heartbeat,  (b)  the  rate  of  breathing,  and  (c)  the  galvanic  skin  resist- 
ance. The  changes  in  the  heartbeat  and  breathing  rates,  but  not  in  the 
galvanic  skin  resistance,  were  much  more  alike  in  the  identical  twins  than 
in  the  fraternals,  indicating  heritability  factors  at  work  in  the  first  two 
responses.  The  grantee  would  like  to  see  more  research  along  this  line 
because  he  believes  that  the  evidence  for  a  genetic  factor  in  some  types  of 
responses  by  the  autonomic  nervous  system  may  also  be  partial  evidence 
for  a  genetic  factor  in  psychosomatic  illness.  The  person  who  naturally 
responds  to  stress  by  an  above-normal  increase  in  his  heart  beat,  Dr, 
Vandenberg  asks-may  he  not  be  the  one  most  likely  to  develop  a  psycho- 
somatic heart  ailment?  Other  investigators  have  suggested  that  persons 
with  different  personalities  develop  different  kinds  of  psychosomatic  com- 
plaints. Hence,  Dr.  Vandenberg  believes,  research  might  well  find  a  close 

514 


relationship  among  illness  and  two  factors  controlled  in  part  by  heredity- 
personality  and  the  activity  of  the  autonomic  nervous  system. 


Fraternal  and  Identical  Twins:  Analyzing  the  Differences 

Since  a  pair  of  identical  twins  have  developed  from  the  same  feitHized 
egg,  they  have  the  same  heredity,  and  any  difference  between  them  must 
have  been  caused  by  environment  alone.  But  a  pair  of  fraternal  twins, 
having  developed  from  two  fertilized  eggs,  have  different  heredities;  hence 
the  differences  between  them  have  been  caused  by  both  heredity  and 
environment.  Through  statistical  analysis  of  these  differences,  investiga- 
tors are  able  to  say  whether  a  given  aspect  of  intelligence  (for  example, 
facility  in  using  numbers)  appears  to  be  strongly  or  weakly  controlled  by 
heredity. 

Like  earlier  researchers,  the  Louisville  gtoup  assumes  that  the  differen- 
ces in  the  environment  of  a  pah-  of  twins  are  roughly  the  same  foi  identi- 
cal as  for  like-sexed  fraternal.  The  investigators  know  that  this  is  not  quite 
true.  For  one  thing,  the  difference  in  the  prenatal  environments  of  identi- 
cal twins,  taken  all  together,  is  probably  greater  because  of  the  greater 
possibility  of  an  imbalance  in  placental  circulation.  For  another,  identical 
twins  are  more  likely  than  fraternals  to  be  treated  alike  by  their  families. 
The  Louisville  studies  may  eventually  help  to  show  whether  or  not  these 
differences  are  significant.  At  the  most,  Dr.  Vandenberg  believes,  they 
make  for  either  a  slight  overestimate  or  a  slight  underestimate  of  hered- 
ity's role. 

In  analyzing  the  differences  between  the  two  types  of  twins,  the  grant- 
ee uses  an  index  based  on  a  statistical  concept  (the  F-ratio)  developed  by 
the  British  statistician,  R.  A.  Fisher.  For  each  test,  he  subtracts  Johnnie's 
score  from  Jimmy's,  Ronald's  from  Donald's,  Ellen's  from  Helen's,  and  so 
on,  Then,  for  each  kind  of  twins,  he  adds  the  squared  differences  and  uses 
the  results  to  get  an  index  of  the  strength  of  the  hereditary  factor.  The 
index,  or  F-ratio,  is  obtained  by  dividing  each  sum  by  the  number  of 
pairs  and  then  dividing  the  figure  for  the  fraternal  twins  by  the  figure  for 
the  identical  twins.  The  result  is  almost  invariably  greater  than  1,  indi- 
cating that  the  wi thin-pair  variance  of  the  fraternal  twins  is  greater  than 
that  of  the  identical  twins.  If  the  ratio  is  statistically  significant-meaning 
that  the  probability  it  could  have  occurred  by  chance  is  very  slight-the 
particular  ability  being  tested  probably  has  a  strong  hereditary  element. 

By  this  technique  the  contribution  of  environment  to  the  expression  of 
a  particular  ability  is  subtracted,  loosely  speaking,  from  the  contribution 
of  heredity.  The  method  is  most  reliable  when  fairly  large  numbers  of 
twins  are  studied.  The  findings  for  a  group,  since  they  give  th 


the  chaiacteristic  is  expressed,  if  at  all,  may  depend  in  large  part  upon  the 
environment.  He  has  found  that  tests  commonly  used  for  measuring  basic 
mental  abilities  give  much  the  same  results  whether  administered  to 
American  college  students  or  to  young  people  who,  though  going  to  col- 
lege in  the  United  States,  have  been  brought  up  in  quite  different  cultural 
backgrounds-South  American  and  Chinese.  This  finding  is  considered  by 
Dr.  Vandenberg  to  be  evidence  that  the  abilities  measured  by  the  tests  are 
cross-cultural  and  perhaps  innate. 

In  spite  of  their  value,  Dr.  Vandenberg  describes  twin  studies  as  only 
"an  economical  first  step  in  human  behavior  genetics,"  They  are  only  a 
fiist  step  because  they  can  be  used  to  deduce  what  traits  are  inherited  but 
not  how.  Eventually,  when  enough  reliable  tests  for  assessing  the  influence 
of  heredity  are  available,  researches  will  have  to  look  for  the  how  by 
undertaking  family  studies.  They  will  have  to  search  for  the  appearance, 
over  generations,  of  behavioial  characteristics  related  to  psychological 
functioning  much  as  medical  geneticists  have  searched  for  evidence  of 
hereditary  factors  in  phenylketonuria,  hemophilia,  diabetes,  and  other 
physical  disorders. 

"If  we  remember  that  a  rather  clear-cut  and,  one  would  think,  rather 
noticeable  defect  such  as  color  blindness  has  been  known  to  science  for 
little  more  than  a  century,"  Dr.  Vandenberg  observes,  "it  does  not  seem 
unreasonable  to  expect  that  we  are  overlooking  other  anomalies,11  As  a 
possible  case  in  point,  he  notes  the  suggestion  by  a  Swedish  investigator 
that  severe  reading  difficulties  may  stem  from  a  hereditary  factor  con- 
u  oiled  by  a  single  gene. 

If  an  inherited  characteristic  is  harmful,  perhaps  we  can  learn  how  to 
prevent  its  full  expression,  as  we  have  learned  how  to  prevent  phenylketo- 
nuria from  leading  still  fuither  to  permanent  mental  retardation. 
Emotional  illness,  however,  is  a  far  more  complex  problem  than 
phenylketonuria,  partly  because  many  genes  may  be  involved  instead  of 
only  one,  and  partly  because  the  biochemical  mechanism  can  be  fully 
activated,  perhaps,  only  by  certain  stressful  environmental  conditions. 

Of  the  present  psychological  and  biochemical  measures,  the  giantec 
believes  we  shall  probably  find  that  no  single  one  is  clearly  related  to  such 
complex  conditions  as  schizophrenia  and  obsessive  compulsive  neurosis. 
More  likely  >  statistical  analyses  of  the  results  of  many  tests  given  to  large 
numbers  of  patients  will  eventually  succeed  in  isolating  a  variety  of  factors 
involved.  Efficient  tests  of  these  factors  can  then  be  developed. 

If  the  influence  on  behavior  of  heredity,  environment,  and  the  two  of 
them  acting  together  is  to  be  clearly  understood,  Dr.  Vandenberg  suggests 
that  we  may  need  at  least  a  nationwide  system  of  storing  information 
about  individuals  and  families.  But  we  can  start  on  a  smaller  scale.  For 
example,  we  can  go  after  proof  of  the  theory  that  schizophrenia  has  a 
rather  strong  hereditary  component.  The  grantee  thinks  that  a  carefully 
planned  10-year  study  of  several  thousand  patients  and  their  family 
backgrounds-a  research  project  in  connection  with  an  existing  service 
project-would  come  up  with  a  definite  answer.  It  might  even  supply  an 
inkling  of  the  genetic  mechanism. 

516 


Tests  of  Intelligence 

It  is  widely  agreed  that  heredity  plays  a  huge  role  in  intelligence,  but 
intelligence  is  many  things.  In  fact,  one  authority  attending  the  recent 
Conference  on  Research  in  Human  Behavior  Genetics  (supported  by  the 
Institute  through  a  grant  to  Dr.  Vandenberg)  reported  that  he  had  pre- 
liminary evidence  of  two  entirely  different  kinds  of  intelligence,  one  de- 
pending mainly  on  heredity  and  the  other  on  culture.  Dr.  Vandenberg 
prefers  to  say  that  certain  of  the  factors  01  abilities  comprising  intelligence 
can  be  developed  through  training  to  a  much  greater  extent  than  otheis. 

Psychologists  do  not  agree  on  the  number  of  diffeient  abilities  contri- 
buting to  intelligence-more  than  100,  by  one  estimate-oi  on  the  exact 
nature  of  many  of  them.  However,  five  of  the  factors  in  the  frequently 
used  commercial  test  referred  to  earlier,  the  Primaiy  Mental  Abilities  test 
(PMA),  are  generally  considered  to  represent  in  fact  distinct  chatacter- 
istics.  This  is  because,  on  the  average,  the  scores  made  in  tests  of  any  one 
of  these  five  abilities-numerical,  verbal,  spatial,  word  fluency,  and 
memory— do  not  conelate  highly  with  the  scores  made  in  tests  of  any  of 
the  others.  As  previously  noted,  Dr.  Vandenberg  has  found  that  the  first 
four  of  these  abilities  are  not  only  independent  but  also  strongly  influ- 
enced by  heredity. 

The  grantee  began  by  administering  the  standard  version  of  the  PMA  as 
an  important  part  of  his  tests,  but  this  year  he  is  using  an  expanded  version 
drawn  from  Dr.  Thurstone's  original  material.  The  new  version  covers 
perhaps  14  separate  abilities.  The  complete  Louisville  battery  includes  a 
number  of  other  "intelligence"  tests,  and  several  tests  of  personality— 
about  three  dozen  tests  in  all,  some  widely  accepted,  otheis  expeiimental. 
The  tests  are  made  up  of  about  1 25  measures,  01  subtests, 

Through  a  method  similar  to  factor  analysis,  the  investigator  can  infer 
whether  or  not  the  differences  between  fraternal  twins  on  a  number  of  the 
tests  can  be  attributed  to  the  same  hereditary  factor.  He  can  also  estimate 
what  percentage  of  the  variation  within  all  the  pairs  of  fraternal  twins  on, 
say,  the  verbal  ability  factor,  is  accounted  for  by  a  given  test.  Thus  he  can 
combine  and  weigh-and  discard  and  add  to-tests  until  hehas  put  together 
the  battery  that  makes  for  the  best  possible  separation  of  fraternal  and 
identical  twins.  In  sum,  when  he  is  considering  many  factors  of  intelli- 
gence and  personality,  he  can  say  which  ones  appear  to  be  under  strong 
genetic  control,  and,  when  he  is  considering  many  tests,  which  ones  most 
clearly  differentiate  between  the  two  types  of  twins,  and  therefore  are  of 
most  value  to  studies  of  the  heritability  of  psychological  characteristics. 

A  number  of  provocative  findings  about  psychological  tests  as  indi- 
cators of  inherited  characteristics  were  turned  up  by  the  University  of 
Michigan  study  with  which  the  grantee  was  connected. 

Close  to  half  of  all  the  psychological  measures  used  in  that  project,  but 
less  than  a  third  of  those  in  the  area  of  personality,  Dr.  Vandenberg  has 
found,  gave  statistically  significant  evidence  that  the  characteristics  being 
measured  had  a  hereditary  component.  Sometimes  the  heritability  esti- 
mate was  strongly  influenced  by  the  type  of  measure  being  used.  For 

517 


instance,  one  vocabulaiy  test  calling  upon  the  subject  himself  to  supply 
the  definitions  yielded  results  showing  a  decided  hereditary  influence, 
while  two  othei  vocabulary  tests,  one  giving  a  choice  of  answers  and  one 
asking  the  students  to  match  words  with  pictures,  produced  opposite 
icsults.  The  Michigan  group  found  half  a  dozen  instances  of  this  sort,  and 
conceivably  would  have  found  more  if  each  ability  or  trait  had  been 
measured  by  two  or  more  tests.  It  also  found  some  characteristics  that 
showed  a  stiong  hereditary  component  but  have  little  or  no  practical 
application-so  far  as  anyone  knows  now.  One  of  these  is  the  ability  to 
trace  a  pattern  while  watching  one's  work  in  the  mirror.  Dr.  Vandenberg 
cites  these  findings  as  evidence  of  the  need  for  wider  experimentation  in 
psychological  testing  if  the  hereditary  elements  of  behavior  are  to  be 
pinned  down.  Tests  that  work  well  in  predicting  success  in  certain  studies 
or  professions,  he  suggests,  are  not  necessarily  the  best  ones  for  research  in 
genetics.  It  may  well  be  that  some  inherited  psychological  differences  are 
lying  undiscovered  because  they  have  no  applied  usefulness  or  because  we 
have  not  been  measuring  them. 

Of  paiticular  interest  to  the  grantee  right  now  are  the  spatial  tests 
because  he  thinks  spatial  visualization,  an  important  ability  in  engineers, 
may  prove  to  have  a  stronger  hereditaiy  element— and  therefore  depend 
less  on  training— than  most  other  facets  of  intelligence.  (Present  tests  of 
this  ability  include  putting  a  puzzle  together  in  the  head,  imagining  what  a 
given  object  would  look  like  if  cut  in  half,  finding  a  given  pattern  among 
other  patterns,  and  saying  where  the  holes  will  occur  in  a  piece  of  paper 
when  it  has  been  folded  several  times  and  punched.)  A  test  of  vocational 
preferences  also  seems  to  get  at  tilings  having  a  hereditary  basis-a  finding 
that  probably  can  be  traced  to  the  influence  on  occupational  choice  of 
tempeiament  and  ability,  which  are  to  some  extent  genetically  governed. 

Another  aspect  of  intelligence,  or  perhaps  of  both  personality  and  intel- 
ligence, seems  to  be  closure,  an  ability  to  round  out  or  complete  things, 
Some  types  of  brain-damaged  persons  lack  it.  In  the  closure  tests  used  by 
Dr.  Vandenberg,  developed  by  the  Canadian,  C.  M.  Mooney,  the  twins  are 
presented  with  incomplete  drawings  of  faces  and  asked  to  identify  each 
face  as  male  or  female  and  say  in  which  direction  it  is  looking.  Though 
closure  is  at  present  not  known  to  be  related  to  any  other  ability,  some 
people  do  better  than  others  on  such  a  test.  Closure  items  have  been 
included  in  intelligence  measures,  Dr,  Vandenberg  explains,  mainly  be- 
cause they  do  not  seem  to  be  influenced  by  verbal  ability,  a  factor 
strongly  affected  by  environment  and  generally  difficult  to  control  for. 
Dr.  Vandenberg  guesses  that  high  scores  on  closure  tests  perhaps  indicate 
some  artistic  ability,  but  he  may  have  to  wait  a  long  time  befoie  lie 
knows. 

In  experimental  use,  as  well,  is  a  test  of  the  long-held  theory  that  some 
people  react  more  to  color  and  others  more  to  form,  the  difference  being 
traceable  to  a  deep-seated  personality  difference.  Preliminary  findings  by 
Dr.  Vandenberg  indicate  that  scores  on  the  form-color  test,  developed  by 
Thurstone,  show  a  marked  hereditary  influence. 

518 


Tests  developed  by  J.  P.  Guilford  to  measure  various  aspects  of  creativ- 
ity are  also  used.  (One  asks  the  student  to  suggest,  among  other  items, 
new  uses  for  old  newspapers  and  two  improvements  each  to  various  social 
institutions,  such  as  marriage.)  Whether  or  not  the  tests  really  do  measure 
creativity  in  the  common  sense  of  the  word  is  in  question.  Pieliminary 
Louisville  findings  indicate  that  whatever  they  measure  is  not  under  strong 
hereditary  control. 

Among  the  measures  is  one  developed  by  the  grantee  himself  while 
working  with  the  Schizophrenia  and  Psychopharmacology  Project  of  the 
University  of  Michigan  and  Ypsilanti  State  Hospital.2  In  this  test  a  person 
looks  at  pictures  of  faces  and  chooses  a  woid  that  best  describes  their 
expressions.  One  of  the  major  characteiistics  of  schizophrenia,  Di. 
Vandenberg  explains,  is  a  loss  of  social  sensitivity  of  social  perception, 
marked  by  a  withdrawal  from  other  persons  and  a  lessened  ability  to 
communicate.  As  used  in  the  Michigan  schizophrenia  study,  the  test  ap- 
parently provided  a  measure  of  this  loss;  schizophrenic  patients  performed 
significantly  worse  than  nonschizophrenic  mental  patients,  while  the  non- 
schizophrenic  patients  did  almost  as  well  as  normal  persons.  Perhaps  the 
schizophrenic  cannot  make  the  kind  of  social  judgment  demanded  by  the 
test  because  he  cannot  bring  himself  to  look  at  the  pictures  long  enough; 
perhaps  some  other  disability  is  involved.  In  any  event,  Dr.  Vandenberg 
thinks  that  the  inability  of  schizophrenics  to  make  such  a  judgment  is 
related  to  their  tendency  toward  withdrawal.  Preliminary  findings  of  the 
Louisville  twin  study  indicate  that  social  sensitivity  as  measured  by  this 
test  is  more  influenced  by  environment  than  heredity. 


Heredity  and  Handedness 

Dr.  Vandenberg  is  much  interested  in  handedness,  a  psychophysiologi- 
cal  characteristic  that  may  well  be  controlled  by  only  a  few  genes.  If  just 
two  alternative  genes  are  involved,  for  right-handedness  and  for  left- 
handedness,  a  person  who  received  two  doses  of  the  one  would  be  right 
handed;  two  doses  of  the  other,  left  handed;  one  close  of  each,  either  right 
or  left  handed,  depending  mostly  on  the  training  received  from  his 
parents.  A  number  of  right-handed  persons,  the  investigator  believes,  are 
perhaps  naturally  left-handed  or  ambidextrous~but  don't  know  it. 

The  biggest  difficulty  in  studying  the  heredity  of  this  trait  is  the  lack  of 
a  good  test  for  natural  handedness.  A  simple  test  for  brain  dominance 
might  be  the  answer,  because  a  person  whose  left  hemisphere  was  shown 
to  be  dominant  might  be  expected  to  be  naturally  right-handed.  But  +hth™ 


illumination  reaches  one  of  the  eyes  through  a  filter  The  shorter  axis 
the  apparent  ellipse  is  greater  when  the  filter  is  held  in  front  of  the  m< 

dominant  eye. 

The  grantee  is  concerned  with  handedness,  he  explains,  because  infr 
mation  about  its  genetic  mechanism  would  increase  interest  Jn-ai 
provide  another  model  for-research  in  the  heritability  of  more  "psych 
logical"  characteristics.  Besides,  it  would  be  useful  in  answering  that  o 
question  about  the  effects  of  forcing  a  naturally  left-handed  person  ] 
become  right-handed.  In  tests  involving  motor  skills,  incidentally,  pc 
formance  with  the  right  hand  generally  shows  a  stronger  hereditary  infli 
ence  than  performance  with  the  left. 

Genetic  Control  of  Physical  Characteristics 

The  Michigan  study,  like  the  Louisville  project,  to  some  extent  wa: 
concerned  with  the  degree  of  hereditary  control  over  physical  as  welt  a( 
psychological  development.  Recently  Dr.  Vandenberg  compared  the 
Michigan  findings  in  this  respect  with  those  of  five  other  twin  studies  since 
1926  and  found  a  high  degree  of  heritability  for  virtually  all  the  measures 
that  had  been  included.  Among  these  were  height,  arm  length,  middle 
finger  length,  leg  length,  foot  length,  chest  girth,  neck  girth,  head  length 
and  breadth  and  girth,  eye  spacing,  nose  height  and  breadth,  face  length, 
and  ear  breadth.  Measures  of  length  generally  had  higher  heritability 
values  than  measures  of  width-perhaps,  the  grantee  notes,  because  tlie 
latter  are  more  affected  by  "such  environmental  excesses"  as  obesity  and 
malnutrition.  Perhaps  for  the  same  reason,  the  studies  did  not  agree  as  to 
the  influence  of  heredity  upon  the  waist  measurement  and  upon  weight, 

When  twin  differences  in  body  measurements  were  related  to  twin  dif- 
ferences on  certain  intelligence  tests,  the  Michigan  group  found  that  the 
larger  the  head,  the  higher  the  test  score.  This  relationship  was  statistically 
significant  among  the  identical  twins  but  weaker  and  not  significant 
among  the  fraternals.  Among  the  general  population,  Dr.  Vandenberg 
points  out,  the  relationship  is  obscured  by  other  factors  and  cannot  be 
observed.  It  is  only  when  differences  in  age,  sex,  background,  and  educa- 
tion are  controlled,  as  they  can  be  in  comparing  twins,  that  the  relation- 
ship shows  up. 

Among  the  fraternal  twins  studied  in  the  Michigan  project,  the  one  who 
had  been  born  first  was  found  usually  to  have  made  the  higher  score,  aiuE 
there  was  a  tendency  among  both  types  of  twins  for  the  one  who  had 
been  born  first  to  have  the  larger  head. 

Some  Research  Needs 

Dr.  Vandenberg  considers  research  looking  toward  the  following  goals* 
some  of  them  indicated  earlier,  to  be  highly  important: 

•  A  better  understanding  of  the  basic  elements  making  up  intelligence 
and  personality. 

520 


»  Better  information  about  the  characteristics  measured  by  present 
psychological  tests. 

•  New  tests  or  combinations  of  tests  that  bring  out  clearly  the  heredi- 
tary elements  in  our  abilities  and  personalities. 

•  Better  tests  of  motor  skills  in  children,  particulaily  those  under  six. 
(At  least  during  babyhood,  the  giantee  observes,  motor  development  and 
mental  development  are  related,  and  he  points  to  work  by  another  NIMH 
grantee  indicating  that  disturbances  of  motor  development  in  infancy  may 
be  followed  by  psychological  disorders,  including  schizophrenia.) 

•  Better  evidence  than  we  now  have  as  to  the  role,  if  any,  played  by 
hereditary  factors  in  schizophrenia  and  other  mental  disorders. 

•  Better  information  on  how  learning  or  training  interacts  with  hered- 
ity in  the  development  of  a  given  characteristic.  (As  an  example  of  how 
this  might  be  obtained,  the  grantee  suggests  that  one  twin  of  a  3-year-old 
pair  of  identical  twins  might  be  given  intensive  training  in  reading,  the 
twins  then  being  followed  up  to  learn  whether  or  not  any  lasting  differ- 
ence resulted.) 

Dr.  Vandenberg  remarks  that  while  he  himself  is  primarily  interested  in 
the  contribution  of  heredity,  complete  understanding  of  how  human  be- 
havior is  controlled  by  genes  will  be  reached  only  through  the  work  of 
scientists  in  many  fields.  Studies  of  the  role  of  the  environment  are  just  as 
important  as  studies  of  heteditary  factors, 

Research  Grants:   MH  6203,  MH  7033,  MH  7708,  MH  7880,  K3-MH  18,382 
References: 

Clark,  Philip  J.,  Vandenberg,  Steven  G.,  and  Proctoi,  Charles  H.  On  the  relationship  of 

scores  on  certain  psychological  tests  with  a  number  of  anthropometric  characters 

and  birth  order  in  twins.  Human  Biology,  33(2),  1961. 
Cohen,  William,  Vandenberg,  Steven  G.,  and  Falkner,  Frank.  Aims  of  Louisville  Twin 

Study.  Report  No.  2,  Child  Development  Unit,  University  of  Louisville,  1962. 
Fish,  Barbara.  The  study  of  motor  development  in  infancy  and  its  relationship  to 

psychological  functioning,  A  merican  Journal  of  Psychiatry,  1 17(12),  1961. 
Button,  H.  Bldon,  Vandenberg,  Steven  G,,  and  Clark,  Philip  J.  The  hereditary  abilities 

study:    selection    of  twins,   diagnosis   of  xygosity  and   program    measurements. 

American  Journal  of  Human  Genetics,  14(1),  1962, 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  Behavioral  methods  for  assessing  neuroses  and  psychoses.  In: 

Uhr  and  Miller,  eds.  Drugs  and  Behavior,  New  York,  Wiley,  I960. 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  The  hereditary  abilities  study:  hereditary  components  in  a 

psychological  test  battery.  American  Journal  of  Human  Genetics,  14(2),  1962. 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  How  "stable"  are  heritability  estimates?  A  comparison  of 

herittibility    estimates   from   six    anthropometric   studies.    American   Journal  of 

Physical  Anthropology,  20(3),  1962. 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  Innate  Abilities,  One  or  Many?  A  New  Method  and  Some 

Results.  Report  No.  3,  Child  Development  Unit,  University  of  Louisville,  1963. 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  Contributions  of  Twin  Research  to  Child  Development.  Report 

No.  5,  Child  Development  Unit,  University  of  Louisville,  1964.  Draft  of  chapter 

from  Lipsitt,  L,  P.,  and  Spiker,  C.  C.,  eds.  Advances  in  Child  Development  and 

Behavior  II,  New  York,  Academic  Press. 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.  Multivariate  analysis  of  twin  differences.  Methods  and  Goals  in 

Human  Behavior  Genetics,  New  York,  Academic  Press,  1964, 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G,  The  Primary  Mental  Abilities  of  South  American  Students,  A 

Comparative  Study  of  the  Stability  of  a  Factor  Structure.  (In  press.) 

521 


Vandenberg,  Steven  G  ,  and  Mattsson,  Eira.  The  interpretation  of  facial  expressions  1 

schizophrenics,  other  mental  patients,  normal  adults  and  children.  Acta  Psvch 

logica,  19,1961 
Vandenberg,  Steven  G.,  Clark,  Philip  J.,  and  Samuels,  Ina.  Psychophysiologicai  Rea 

tions  of  Twins-  Hentabihty  Estimates  of  Galvanic  Skin  Resistance,  Heartbeat  at. 

Breathing  Rates.  (In  press.) 


Investigator: 

Irwin  Feinberg,  M.D.* 

New  York  Downstate  Medical  Center 

Brooklyn,  N.Y. 

Co -contributors: 

Howard  Roffwarg,  M.D., 

Montefiore  Hospital,  New  York  City 

H,  W.  Agnew,  Jr., 

University  of  Florida 

Anthony  Kales,  M.D., 

University  of  California  at  Los  Angeles 

D.  R,  Hawkins,  M.D., 

University  of  Virginia 

Arthur  Parmelee,  M.D., 

University  of  California  at  Los  Angeles 

Prcprared  by: 

Gay  Luce 


Background 

From  the  darkness  of  the  womb  the  newborn  child  only  slowly 
emerges:  As  if  incapable  of  plunging  into  consciousness  for  too  long  at 
once,  he  sleeps  and  awakens  in  biief  alternations.  At  the  other  end  of  life, 
as  if  reluctant  to  let  go  of  consciousness  for  too  long  at  one  time,  the  aged 
brain  shows  a  sleep  that  is  punctuated  with  awakenings.  The  depth  of 
infant  sleep  and  the  alterations  brought  by  youth,  adulthood,  and  age 
reveal,  like  indirect  mirrors,  changes  that  inevitably  take  place  in  the 
nervous  system  during  the  span  of  a  lifetime. 

At  birth  the  nervous  system  is  incompletely  developed,  The  brain 
triples  in  weight  after  birth,  reaching  almost  adult  proportions  by  about 
age  6.  During  these  preadolescent  years  the  brain  has  its  greatest  plasticity 
and  presumably  the  greatest  capacity  for  learning.  The  plasticity  of  the 
brain  can  be  inferred  by  the  ability  of  very  young  brains  to  transfer  such 
functions  as  speech  after  significant  damage  or  surgery.  During  adoles- 
cence this  plasticity  diminishes,  and  hormonal  events  make  their  great 
impact  on  the  sexually  maturing  human  being.  Later,  after  the  plateau  of 
maturity,  the  middle-aged  hormonal  system  again  begins  to  change.  An 


*Now  at  Veteran's  Hospital,  San  Francisco,  California 

523 


individual  may  then  begin  to  notice  in  himself  a  trend  that  has  actu; 
transpired  throughout  his  adulthood:  It  has  become  more  difficult  for  J 
to  change  his  habits,  to  learn  a  language,  to  acquire  the  skill  of  a  n 
sport,  or  adopt  a  new  profession.  Nor  is  he  sleeping  as  soundly  as 
youth.  In  old  age,  finally,  this  trend  accelerates,  and  the  individual  beg 
to  find  that  he  can  no  longer  practice  his  old  skills  and  no  longer 
members  what  once  he  knew.  His  sleep,  punctuated  by  wakeful  momeii 
knits  night  and  day  together  in  a  prolongation  of  consciousness. 

The  relationships  between  the  underlying  brain  physiology  and  lifetii 
behavioral  development  are  now  being  explored  by  using  the  electi 
encephalogram  (EEC)  of  sleep  to  depict  the  sleep  patterns  of  each  a| 
from  birth  to  senescence. 

The  overall  picture  of  lifetime  sleep,  as  compiled  by  Dr.  Feinberg  ar 
his  associates,  also  includes  research  conducted  by  other  NIMH  grantee 
who  have  concentrated  upon  particular  age  groups.  In  correlating  Hfetiir 
behavioral  changes  with  sleep  patterns,  the  researchers  expect  that  the; 
sleep-behavioral  relationships  may  offer  clues  about  the  biological  functio 
of  sleep  and  its  various  stages.  Knowledge  of  sleep  patterns  is,  moreovei 
of  clinical  importance.  Dr.  Marvin  Schultz  of  UCLA  has  demonstratei 
that  sleep  patterns  can  now  be  used  to  confirm  very  early  diagnoses  o 
illness  such  as  hypo  thy  roidism  or  of  retardation  in  infants,  A  knowledge 
of  sleep  patterns  is  only  beginning  to  reveal  why  elderly  people  complaii 
of  insomnia,  and  what,  indeed,  this  insomnia  looks  like  and  augurs.  At 
understanding  of  nighttime  insomnia  and  delirious  wanderings  in   the 
senile  may  finally  lead  to  the  development  of  rational  therapy,  rather  thar 
the  current  practice  of  liberal  and  unconsidered  drug  use  for  insomnia  and 
of  institutionalization  for  the  senile.  Base  line  studies  of  sleep,  at  different 
ages,  now  offer  a  yardstick  against  which  researchers  are  looking  at  the 
effects  of  hypnotics  and  other  drugs,  and  from  which  they  are  judging  the 
impressions  the  various  illnesses  mark  upon  nighttime  sleep.  The  base  line 
of  sleep  patterns— for  life-as  compiled  by  these  investigators  will  un- 
doubtedly be  refined  again  in  the  near  future.  It  is  nonetheless  the  founda- 
tion, the  bedrock,  on  which  many  clinical  studies  of  sleep  will  increasingly 
rely. 

The  formulation  presented  here  he  is  not  solely  the  substance  of  one 
laboratory,  but  includes  the  data  of  other  research  teams,  and  offers  a 
reformulation  of  the  age-sleep  picture. 


The  Sleeping  Population 

For  half  a  dozen  years,  many  infants  born  in  the  bright  new  hospital  at 
UCLA  have  been  subjects  for  Dr,  Arthur  Parmelee  and  his  associates, 
while  other  infants  who  were  born  at  Columbia-Presbyterian  Hospital  in 
New  York  were  observed  by  Dr,  Howard  Roffwarg  and  his  associates.  In 
one  of  the  laboratories  at  UCLA,  infants  are  being  followed  inio  the 
preschool  and  early  school  years,  with  sleep  records  and  testing  at  regular 
intervals.  The  data  contributing  to  the  base  line  for  childhood  have  been 

524 


collected  for  a  variety  of  reasons  For  instance,  Di .  Anthony  Kales  and  his 
associates  compared  normal  and  somnambulistic  childien,  while  Dr.  David 
Hawkins,  then  at  the  University  of  North  Carolina,  and  Drs,  Chester  Pierce 
and  Roy  Whitman  of  Cincinnati  were  interested  in  enuresis.  Dr,  David 
Foulkes  has  studied  sleep  in  young  school  children,  moved  by  particular 
interest  in  their  dreams.  In  the  course  of  buigeonmg  sleep  research,  adoles- 
cents, young  adults,  and  middle-aged  people  have  become  the  subjects  of 
diverse  studies.  Dr.  Wilse  Webb  and  H.  W.  Agnew,  Jr.,  at  the  University  of 
Florida,  and  Dr.  Anthony  Kales  and  his  associates  at  UCLA  have  studied 
normal  sleep  in  some  elderly  subjects  for  the  purpose  of  obtaining  a  norm. 
Dr.  Charles  Fisher  at  Mt.  Sinai,  and  Dr.  Ismet  Karacan  now  of  the  Univer- 
sity of  Florida,  were  correlating  the  occurrence  of  erections  with  rapid- 
eye-inovement  sleep  throughout  the  lifetime,  and  were  thus  moved  to 
examine  the  sleep  of  quite  elderly  men. 

This  laboratory  conducted  its  own  studies  of  young  adults,  normal 
older  people,  and  deteriorated  elderly  people  out  of  an  interest  in  the 
relation  between  sleep  patterns,  intellectual  function,  and  age.  As  there 
began  to  be  data  of  disparate  sorts  from  laboratories  all  around  the 
country  and  also  in  Europe,  the  investigators  were  challenged  by  the  need 
for  a  summary  picture,  a  lifeline  of  sleep.  They  added  to  their  own  data  a 
study  of  sleep  in  children  who  were  5  to  10  years  old.  This  filled  in  the 
picture  of  sleep  at  a  time  of  life  when  the  brain  is  physically  almost 
mature,  yet  at  its  most  plastic. 

It  is  difficult  to  obtain  normative  data  on  human  sleep,  and  while  a 
lifetime  scale  of  sleep  patterns  promises  to  become  an  invaluable  diag- 
nostic yardstick,  it  will  be  hard  to  acquire.  The  difficulty  in  amassing 
such  a  yardstick  can  be  seen  from  the  human  investment  in  a  single  study. 
Dr.  Feinberg  and  his  associates  have  given  the  better  part  of  4  years  to 
their  study.  Yet  they  studied  only  38  normal  people  and  15  abnormal 
elderly  persons.  The  age  span  was  5-96.  It  would  be  hard  to  make  these  38 
people  evenly  represent  those  91  years.  Ideally,  a  graph  of  changing  sleep 
patterns  would  evolve  from  records  taken  from  several  normal  people,  one 
group  of  subjects  for  each  year  in  the  lifespan.  Discounting  the  time  it 
takes  to  find  and  acclimatize  subjects,  discounting  the  tediousness  of  con- 
secutive nights  of  sleep  recording,  it  would  probably  take  20  man-years  to 
run  the  ideal  study  of  400-odd  people-five  for  each  year  under  study. 
Some  researchers  are  indeed  studying  numbers  of  subjects  representing  a 
few  years.  For  obvious  reasons  there  is  no  wholesale  endeavor  to  encom- 
pass the  lifetime.  The  approximate  graphs  drawn  from  small  and  scattered 
samples  are  apparently  adequate  to  outline  the  relation  between  sleep 
patterns  and  age. 


The  Volunteers  of  This  Study 

The  young  adults  and  elderly  patients  were  studied  at  St.  Blizabeths-a 
huge  Federal  mental  hospital  located  in  the  southeast  portion  of  Washing- 
ton, D.C.  There,  a  special  ward  was  established  within  a  modern  building, 

525 


where  patients  and  noimal  outsiders  might  live  while  their  nightly  sleep 
was  being  studied.  Nine  young  men  and  six  young  women  were  recruited 
from  among  the  hospital  nurses  and  aides  at  the  hospital  and  persuaded  lo 
sleep  in  the  laboratory  for  five  or  six  nights  in  succession,  and  to  comply 
with  rules  by  not  drinking  alcohol  or  napping  on  those  days.  These  people 
weie  between  1 9  and  36  yeais  of  age. 


Normal  Elderly 

Nine  men  and  six  women  who  were  between  65  and  95  years  old-ati 
average  age  of  77-were  studied  on  a  volunteer  ward  at  the  large  modern 
hospital  on  the  NIH  campus  in  Bethesda,  the  Clinical  Center.  These  un- 
usual people  were  members  of  a  club  called  the  Fossils,  a  wry  veision  of 
the  Golden  Age  clubs  for  retired  and  elderly  persons.  In  contrast  with  the 
young  adults,  these  people  were  comfortable  suburbanites,  retired  profes- 
sionals, and  generally  cultured  and  highly  educated  people.  Nobody  has 
been  able  to  ascertain  that  educational  level  in  any  way  alters  sleep  pat- 
terns, but  the  good  health  of  these  older  people  may  indeed  be  attribut- 
able to  their  social  class.  This  group  was  thoroughly  screened  for  even 
premonitory  signs  of  impaiiment,  and  each  individual  was  brought  into 
the  research  ward  for  5  days  of  acclimatization  before  the  sleep  studies 
began.  These  people  also  observed  the  no-napping  and  no-alcohol  rules. 


Chronic  Brain  Syndrome 

Although  this  group,  studied  at  St.  Elizabeths,  was  matched  for  age 
with  the  Betliesdans,  it  presented  a  sad  contrast  between  a  healthy  old  age 
and  old  age  attended  by  severe  brain  impairment.  These  were  intellec- 
tually damaged  people,  some  of  them  professionals  retired  from  exceed- 
ingly responsible  jobs,  others  were  firemen  and  small  businessmen.  At  the 
time  of  the  study  they  were  incompetent  to  take  care  of  themselves,  sonic 
wandered  around  in  a  disoriented  manner,  and  all  needed  institutional 
care.  Most  of  them  were  not  comparable  with  their  healthy  Bethesda  peers 
in  either  education  or  economic  status,  although  the  research  team  had 
tried  very  hard  to  recruit  volunteers  of  equivalent  cultural  status. 


Children 

Eight  boys  and  girls,  aged  4'/£  to  61/2  and  9  to  10  years,  slept  in  the 
laboratory  atop  the  Downstate  Medical  Center's  large  new  hospital,  a 
laboratory  that  is  often  referred  to  as  the  Dream  Lab.  The  children  were 
brought  by  their  parents  to  the  laboratory  before  bedtime,  coming  on 
several  nights  to  accustom  themselves  to  the  laboratory,  and  for  three 
nights  of  recording. 

526 


All  of  the  subjects,  old  and  young,  weie  carefully  and  lullingly  deco- 
rated with  electiodes  around  the  scalp  and  face  (in  standardized  place- 
ment) before  letiring.  They  were  whed  up  to  the  electroencephalograph 
amplifier  system,  through  a  cable  system  permitting  them  to  sleep  in  a 
private  room,  where  they  were  undisturbed  by  the  comings  and  goings  of 
researchers  in  the  control  room.  In  each  hospital,  the  undeviating  hospital 
routine  dictated  the  hour  at  which  the  sleep  subjects  had,  perforce,  to  rise. 
At  St.  Elizabeths  the  rising  hour  was  6:30,  while  at  NIH  and  Downstate 
the  subjects  slept  until  7  or  7:30. 


The  High  Water  Marks 

Throughout  the  night,  changing  brain  waves  and  physiological  functions 
leave  a  sea  of  data,  marked  by  only  a  few  distinguishable  tides,  rhythms 
that  are  recurrent  and  obvious.  Within  this  sea  of  data,  the  investigators 
selected  certain  intervals  to  act  as  the  high  water  marks  of  the  night.  How 
long  did  the  volunteer  sleep?  How  often  did  he  awaken  from  sleep?  How 
[ong  did  it  take  him  to  fall  asleep  when  the  lights  went  out?  How  long  did 
it  take  to  reach  the  first  rapid-eye-movement  period,  and  to  show  not  only 
the  irregular  low-voltage  brain  waves  but  also  an  eye  movement?  How 
much  of  the  night  did  he  spend  in  REM  sleep,  and  how  much  in  slow-wave 
sleep? 

The  investigators  subdivided  the  usual  EEG  categories  of  sleep  into 
more  refined  intervals  which  they  defined  quantitatively.  When  they 
looked  for  the  amounts  of  deep  delta  sleep  (stages  3  and  4),  they  would 
count  the  number  of  slow  waves  of  at  least  50  microvolts  in  a  20-second 
interval:  They  called  it  stage  3  if  there  were  between  1 0  and  1 5  waves,  and 
stage  4  if  the  interval  contained  more  than  15  such  waves,  They  also 
examined  the  EEG  for  bursts  of  activity  that  visually  resemble  wire 
spindles,  bursts  that  last  about  half  a  second  or  moie,  and  contain  12—14 
cycles  a  second.  This  is  a  configuration  that  does  not  occur  in  the  EEG  of 
sleep  until  after  about  age  3  months.  It  is  reduced  or  absent  in  old  age. 

The  investigators  looked  at  intervals  of  rapid-eye-movement  sleep  in 
several  ways.  Within  the  REM  period  are  many  moments  when  the  eyes 
are  not  moving  at  all,  and  the  EEG  shows  a  pattern  of  light,  low-voltage 
sleep.  The  researchers  looked  at  density  of  eye  movements.  They  meas- 
ured the  intervals  of  stage  1  sleep  preceding  and  succeeding  the  eye 
movements,  and  the  percentage  of  the  night's  sleep  spent  in  this  variegated 
REM  period.  They  measured  the  night's  total  REM  sleep.  They  looked  at 
the  amount  of  time  an  individual  slept,  next  to  the  amount  of  time  he 
spent  in  bed.  They  looked  at  the  periodicity  of  nightly  events-the  timing 
of  recurrent  stages  of  EEG.  Their  breakdown  differed  from  that  of  various 
colleagues,  but  the  emerging  picture  from  various  laboratories  has  been 
amazingly  consonant. 

527 


The  infant 

In  J955  Drs.  Eugene  Aserinsky  and  Nathaniel  Kleitman  observed  that 
infants  in  their  first  7  months  alternated  predictably  between  active  and 
quiet  periods  in  sleep,  roughly  every  hour.  Eight  years  later  Dr.  Howard 
Roffwarg  was  able  to  report  in  considerable  detail  what  happens  during  an 
infant's  sleep.  Using  tiny  sensois  they  were  able  to  track  the  heart  rate, 
the  brain  waves,  the  respiration,  and  muscle  tone  of  the  newborn  baby  as 
he  slept.  A  year  later  Dr.  Arthur  H.  Parmelee  and  his  colleagues  at  UCLA 
confirmed  these  findings  from  their  own  detailed  study  of  infants  begin- 
ning on  the  first  day  of  life.  During  the  quiet  phase  of  sleep  the  infants 
showed  a  tegular  respiration  and  scarcely  any  body  movements,  but  dur- 
ing the  active  sleep  that  has  resemblance  to  REM  sleep,  the  infants  smiled, 
kicked,  grimaced,  made  eye  movements  and  sudden  gestures  with  their 
arms  or  legs. 

For  about  his  first  3  months,  an  infant  naps.  Only  slowly  does  he  begin 
to  sleep  primarily  at  night.  As  infants  have  been  recorded  and  observed 
around  the  clock,  during  their  first  week  of  life,  it  was  clear  that  there 
were  already  some  important  individual  differences  in  the  amount  of  sleep 
and  the  length  of  the  baby's  cycle.  One  infant  would  sleep  for  40-mmute 
intervals,  16  times  a  day,  while  another  would  sleep  in  14  longer  periods. 
The  UCLA  laboratory  has  studied  about  25  infants  between  birtli  and  3 
months,  and  found  that  in  this  period  babies  generally  show  a  sleep  cycle 
of  about  40-45  minutes,  which  lengthens  as  the  infant  matures. 

Although  many  people  imagine  that  infants  sleep  most  of  the  time  and 
that  they  are  awake  more  as  they  mature,  this  is  not  really  the  major 
change  in  the  first  2  years  of  life.  A  newborn  infant  who  sleeps  1 4  hours  a 
day  in  his  first  weeks,  may  still  be  sleeping  about  1 2  hours  a  day  at  age  2 
years.  The  primary  change  is  in  the  distribution  of  his  sleep  and  waking: 
He  begins  to  sleep  continuously  through  the  night  and  be  awake  by  clay, 
in  the  almost  24-hour  rhythmic  pattern  of  his  parents. 

The  EGG  stages  of  sleep  are  shifting  in  their  periodicity  too.  The  brain 
waves  of  a  premature  or  full-term  infant  are  not  so  coherent  and  defined 
as  those  of  the  child  or  adult.  EGG  studies  by  Dr.  Parmelee  and  his 
associates  at  UCLA,  and  independent  studies  by  Drs.  Howard  Roffwarg, 
Joseph  Muzio,  and  William  Dement  suggest  that  the  infant  spends  about 
one-third  of  his  existence  in  a  state  resembling  REM  sleep.  Premature 
babies  show  even  greater  proportions  of  this  sleep.  In  the  first  days  of  life, 
this  stage  occupies  about  50  percent  of  the  baby's  sleep,  declining  as  the 
infant's  nervous  system  matures,  The  infant,  like  no  normal  adult,  will  fat! 
directly  from  waking  into  this  rapid-eye-movement  sleep,  so  characterized 
by  dreaming  in  adults,  and  by  subtle  facial  expressions  and  extraordinary 
physical  activity  on  the  part  of  infants.  Children  over  IH  or  2  years  will 
sleep  for  almost  3  hours,  and  adults  will  sleep  for  about  an  hour  before 
falling  into  a  rapid-eye-movement  state.  Although  children  and  adults  may 
speak  out,  grimace,  or  exhibit  certain  twitches  of  muscles,  they  do  not 
wave  their  arms  or  legs,  nor  thrash  about  wildly  during  their  REM  dreams 
as  do  infants.  Signs  of  exaggerated  activity,  like  those  of  the  infant  during 

528 


REM  sleep,  are  highly  abnormal  in  adults.  Indeed,  the  only  adults  who 
have  exhibited  as  much  REM  sleep  as  a  newborn  infant  have  been  drug 
addicts,  alcoholics  during  withdrawal,  or  people  who  were  experimentally 
deprived  of  this  sleep  stage  on  prior  nights.  Some  of  these  people  have 
been  recorded  during  a  night  that  was  half  given  over  to  REM  sleep. 

Children  studied  by  Dr.  Feinbeig  and  his  associates,  like  children 
studied  by  Dr.  Roffwarg,  et  al.,  Dr.  Kales  et  al.,  H.  W.  Agnew,  Jr.  et  al., 
spent  around  25  percent  of  their  sleeping  time  in  the  REM  state;  this  was 
about  the  same  proportion  as  the  young  adults.  The  amount  of  REM  sleep 
declined  slightly  in  the  older  people,  and  noticeably  in  the  patients  with 
chronic  brain  syndrome,  but  without  greatly  changing  the  relative  propor- 
tions of  REM  and  non-REM  sleep.  The  impaired  old  people  sometimes 
reacted  to  REM  sleep  in  the  manner  of  an  alcoholic  suffering  withdrawal 
symptoms.  Three  of  the  patients  often  awakened  from  a  REM  dream  with 
a  start.  They  would  try  to  pull  off  their  electrodes  and  leave,  saying  they 
had  to  make  a  train,  be  at  work  on  time,  01  meet  a  business  associate, 
They  were  in  effect  delirious  and  had  to  be  restrained  from  racing  out  of 
the  building  to  do  the  prosaic  enands  of  their  dreams. 

It  has  been  conjectured  that  the  intensity  of  the  dream  experience  may 
be  detected  by  the  density  of  rapid  eye  movements.  When  a  person  has 
been  deprived  of  REM  sleep,  he  compensates  later  by  indulging  in  more 
than  usual  REM  sleep  with  more  intense  eye  movements.  The  paucity  of 
eye  movements  in  the  REM  sleep  of  retarded  children  is  one  bit  of  data 
suggesting  that  the  eye-movement  activity  of  REM  sleep  may  be  related  to 
brain  metabolism. 

Curiously  enough,  the  direction  of  the  dreamer's  eye  movements 
changes  with  age.  Newborn  babies  and  infants  make  many  more  vertical 
eye  movements  than  horizontal  movements  in  their  active  sleep.  Elderly 
people  and  senile  patients  were  found  to  make  mainly  horizontal  eye 
movements  during  their  REM  experiences.  Children  and  young  adults 
showed  both,  although  their  eye  movements  have  been  reported  to  be 
primarily  horizontal  by  Dr.  J.  Antrobus. 

Memory  of  the  REM  experience  also  seems  to  change  with  age,  although 
pure  memory  is  hardly  what  the  sleep  researcher  determines  when  he 
awakens  a  sleepy  child  or  an  old  person  and  asks  what  he  was  thinking. 
Neither  young  children  nor  old  people  seem  to  recall— or  to  be  able  to 
recite— dream  experiences  as  well  as  young  adults. 

This  has  been  an  observation  in  a  recent  study  by  Drs.  Edwin  Kahn  and 
Charles  Fisher  at  Mount  Sinai  Hospital  in  New  York  City.  In  contrast  to 
Dr.  Feinberg's  study,  they  found  no  correlation  between  age  and  the 
amount  of  REM  sleep  in  11  old  men.  This  discrepancy  may  be  due  to 
differences  in  sampling  or  in  procedure.  The  elderly  gentlemen  studied  by 
Drs.  Kahn  and  Fisher  slept  with  a  strain  gauge  attached  to  the  penis.  They 
still  showed  some  degree  of  the  erections  that  are  seen  in  all  males  during 
REM  periods,  but  when  they  were  awakened  from  REM  sleep  they  had  far 
less  dream  recall  than  a  young  adult.  The  investigators  conjectured  that 
vitality  and  psychological  vigor  may  determine  the  amount  of  REM  sleep 
obtained  at  a  later  age  in  life. 

529 


Rapicl-eye-movenient  sleep  has  been  linked  with  activity  in  a  primit 
portion  of  the  lower  brain,  the  pons.  It  is  not  surprising  that  brain  st< 
activity  might  be  dominant  in  premature  babies,  whose  brains  are  in  t 
early  stages  of  development.  It  has  been  speculated  that  this  rudimenta 
braJn  activity,  occurring  in  the  womb,  constitutes  a  state  that  would  n 
be  dreaming  in  an  adult's  sense,  but  which  stimulates  brain  experiences 
preparation  for  life.  Dr.  Feinberg  and  other  researchers  have  specula  t 
that  the  high  amount  of  dreaming  found  during  early  childhood  is  relati 
to  the  enormous  amount  that  a  child  must  learn- for  in  early  life  the  bra 
must  absorb  more  than  it  ever  will  later.  One  emerging  theory  about  RE 
sleep  suggests  that  tliis  is  a  time  when  newly  learned  material  is  stored  ar 
filed  in  the  brain. 

The  rhythm  of  infant  sleep  and  waking  has  been  traced  back  into  tl 
womb  in  a  recording  from  a  mother  during  her  sleep.  Until  about 
months'  gestation,  the  activity  of  the  fetus  was  often  changing.  After 
months,  there  was  a  regular  44-minute  activity  period,  with  a  ronght 
half-hour  rest  in  between.  After  delivery,  the  infant  showed  the  sam 
cycle  of  activity  during  his  first  days. 

The  newborn  infant's  sleep  rhythm  only  faintly  resembles  that  of  cliil 
dren  and  adults,  although  the  EEC  of  REM  sleep  even  in  very  youn; 
infants  bears  a  striking  resemblance  to  REM  and  stage  2  in  adults.  Tin 
sleep  records  have  been  compaied  by  three  main  criteria:  The  amount 
and  percentages  of  REM  sleep  and  slow-wave  sleep;  the  length  of  interval1 
such  as  the  transition  from  waking  into  active  sleep;  the  periodicity  of  tlu 
complete  sleep  cycle. 

Striking  changes  occur  in  the  first  3  months,  as  the  proportion  ol 
rapid-eye-movement  sleep  declines  in  the  infant  and  the  baby  begins  to 
sleep  at  night.  The  child  of  6  months  or  a  year  spends  about  a  third  of  his 
sleep  in  REM  dreaming.  By  age  2  this  has  dropped  to  about  a  quarter. 
Somewhere  between  3  and  5  years  the  proportion  of  REM  sleep  falls  to 
about  20—25  percent,  which  is  close  to  the  norm  throughout  much  of 
adulthood.  The  total  amount  of  sleep  obtained  by  a  child  decreases  very 
slowly. 

Drs.  J.  Mendels  and  D.  R.  Hawkins,  at  the  University  of  North  Carolina, 
have  begun  an  extensive  study  of  children  from  2  to  16  years  in  which 
they  expect  to  study  five  children  for  each  year.  They  find,  in  their  first 
recordings,  that  there  is  a  slow  diminution  of  actual  sleep  time,  propor- 
tionately diminishing  the  time  spent  in  each  stage  of  sleep,  but  without 
changing  the  proportion  to  the  total  night  of  sleep.  A  very  noticeable 
difference  between  the  sleep  of  young  children  and  adults  is  the  time 
spent  in  a  deep  stage-4  oblivion. 

A  child  not  only  obtains  more  of  this  slow-wave  sleep,  but  he  is  much 
slower  to  awaken.  While  the  old  person  will  awaken  from  sleep  with 
alacrity,  almost  with  a  nervous  promptness,  the  child  tends  to  be  a 
somnambulist  for  quite  a  while,  fumbling  around  sleepily  if  awakened, 
and  often  drifting  back  to  sleep  while  on  his  feet  or  in  the  lap  of  a  parent. 
Delta  sleep,  which  occupies  much  of  the  first  3  to  4  hours  of  a  2-year-old's 
sleep,  is  the  stage  from  which  a  child  will  sleepwalk,  and  this  phenomenon, 

530 


which  is  not  uncommon  among  2-  and  3-year-olds,  becomes  progressively 
more  rare  with  age— until  a  new  and  different  kind  of  somnambulism 
emerges  in  senility.  The  senile  person  perambulates  out  of  REM  sleep:  He 
has  no  stage-4  sleep. 

The  youngster  between  2  and  10  is  particularly  hard  to  awaken  early  in 
the  night  when  he  is  drowned  in  the  depths  of  delta  sleep.  His  stage  4 
sleep  differs  from  that  of  his  parents  not  only  in  the  subjective  experi- 
ence: The  delta  waves  of  a  child  are  of  ex h emery  high  amplitude.  A 
5-year-old  spends  around  2  hours  a  night  in  this  deepest  forgetfulness, 
whereas  the  adolescent  spends  only  about  75-80  minutes.  The  fiist  nightly 
REM  period  may  not  appear  for  3  to  4  houis  in  a  7-year-old,  whereas  it 
will  arrive  within  50-70  minutes  after  an  adult  has  fallen  asleep.  The  speed 
with  which  a  person  reaches  his  first  REM  dream  after  falling  asleep 
becomes  more  rapid  as  he  matures.  The  child  begins  to  show  an  adult 
pattern  of  REM  latency  around  mid-adolescence. 

When  H.  W.  Agnew,  Jr.,  and  Drs.  Wilse  B.  Webb  and  R.  L.  Williams 
compared  10-year-olds  with  teenage  youths,  they  found  only  slight  dif- 
ferences in  their  sleep  records.  The  10-year-olds  slept  longer-averaging 
around  9*/2  hours.  The  teenagers  slept  about  2  hours  less.  The  10-year-olds 
showed  a  higher  amplitude  activity  on  their  EGG's  than  the  teenagers.  If> 
as  some  researches  speculate,  the  power  lepresented  by  the  amplitude  of 
a  biain  wave  is  related  to  the  intensity  of  sleep,  then  it  may  be  possible  to 
watch  the  intensity  of  stage-4  sleep  decline  steadily  over  a  lifetime. 


Young  Adults 

Sleep  has  been  well  documented  for  the  young  man  between  20  and  28. 
There  are,  to  be  sure,  notable  differences  among  individuals,  but  the  gen- 
eral picture  of  a  night's  sleep  in  the  twenties  is  by  now  familiar.  The 
so-called  average  person  falls  asleep,  taking  about  7—15  minutes.  He  drifts 
down  through  lighter  sleep,  stages  1 ,  2,  3,  into  stage  4.  Then  he  drifts  back 
up  toward  light  sleep  and  enters  a  REM  dream  in  70  minutes  from  the 
time  he  went  to  sleep.  The  first  REM  dream  is  apt  to  be  short,  in  the 
neighborhood  of  10  minutes.  Once  more,  the  person  dives  into  the  depths, 
rising  again  so  that  after  about  75  minutes  he  is  ready  to  dream  at  greater 
length.  The  dive  into  slow-wave  sleep,  which  happens  about  five  times, 
goes  less  deep  in  the  last  cycle  of  night.  The  four  last  REM  periods  of  the 
night  run  about  20-25  minutes,  but  the  intervals  between  REM  sleeps 
have  diminished. 

Delta  sleep,  and  particularly  stage  4,  provides  a  visible  demarcation 
between  childhood,  youth,  and  that  point  at  which  adulthood  slips  over 
the  edge  into  old  age.  Even  the  young  adult  receives  far  less  delta  sleep 
than  he  did  before  adolescence  or  during  adolescence.  His  delta  sleep,  as 
seen  on  the  EEC,  is  much  lower  in  amplitude.  His  complete  sleep  cycles 
run  between  70  and  90  minutes,  yet  within  the  basic  pattern  lurk  many 
variations. 

531 


In  one  preliminary  study  of  good  and  poor  sleepers  there  appeared  I 
be  unmistakable  physiological  differences  between  them,  differences  i 
body  temperature,  pulse,  vasoconstriction,  amount  of  REM  sleep,  and  t] 
distribution  of  stage  4,  as  well  as  marked  personality  differences.  On 
young  adult  in  the  comparative  study  of  Dr.  Fein  berg  and  his  associate 
showed  no  delta  sleep  at  all,  yet  seemed  normal  during  his  waking  hour; 

For  the  most  part,  however,  a  number  of  lesearchers  observe  that  th 
EEC  amplitude  of  stage-4  sleep  declines  with  age.  Not  only  is  there  les 
stage  4  until  it  disappears  altogether,  but  it  diminishes  in  what  might  bi 
called  intensity,  and  which  would  be  defined  on  the  EEC  as  prolongec 
trains  of  high  amplitude  delta  waves.  The  duration  of  stage  4  diminishe; 
toward  a  vanishing  point  that  may  occur  as  early  as  the  late  thirties  01 
early  forties. 

The  decline  of  stage  4  has  been  observed  by  Dr.  Wilse  B.  Webb  and 
H.  W.  Agnew,  Jr.,  at  the  University  of  Florida,  They  have  observed  that  as 
delta  sleep  declines,  sleep  may  become  less  continuous,  and  its  fabric 
becomes  punctured  with  awakenings.  At  about  age  45  people  are  likely  to 
awaken  three  times  in  a  night.  People  who  were  allowed  to  go  on  sleeping 
in  the  morning  in  the  Florida  laboratory  until  they  were  "slept  out" 
found  themselves  awakening  five  and  six  times  toward  the  end,  when  their 
sleep  was  largely  the  light  stage  2  and  REM.  The  absence  of  stage4  sleep 
in  the  aged  and  increased  awakenings  were  also  observed  at  UCLA  by  Drs. 
Anthony  Kales  and  Allen  Jacobson  in  their  study  of  elderly  people, 

As  Dr.  Feinberg  and  his  associates  compared  children,  young  adults, 
and  elderly  people,  they  found  that  an  adult,  once  beyond  adolescence, 
does  not  sleep  longer  at  age  20-30  than  his  counterpart  of  70-80.  The 
young  adults  recorded  at  St.  Elizabeths,  who  were  between  19  and  36, 
slept  about  the  same  amount  of  time  as  their  counterparts  in  Bethesda, 
the  aged  members  of  the  Fossils  Club.  However,  they  took  less  time  falling 
asleep  than  either  the  normal  older  counterpart  or  the  patient  with  brain 
damage.  They  did  not  awaken  so  much  at  night.  The  older  person 
obtained  an  equivalent  amount  of  sleep  only  by  staying  in  bed  a  longer 
time.  The  person  with  chronic  brain  damage,  who  took  a  long  time  falling 
asleep,  did  not  really  obtain  as  much  sleep  as  his  peer  in  age.  The  normal 
older  person  in  this  study  awakened  so  often  that  he  spent  17  percent  of 
his  time  in  bed  wakeful.  The  young  adult  spent  his  supine  hours  asleep, 
but  the  institutionalized  person  with  symptoms  of  senility  lay  awake  fora 
third  of  his  time  in  bed. 

The  studies  conducted  by  Dr.  Feinberg  and  his  associates  enumerated 
many  of  the  differences  between  the  young  and  old  that  make  anecdotes 
in  the  conversations  of  large  families.  For  instance,  it  is  common  for  an 
older  person  to  rise  and  go  to  the  bathroom  once,  or  even  more  often, 
during  the  night.  Young  adults  do  not,  unless  they  are  in  exceptional 
states:  for  instance,  women  in  late  pregnancy  find  their  sleep  interrupted 
and  do  visit  the  bathroom.  A  UCLA  study  indicates  that  some  enuretic 
youngsters  are  never  even  awakened  by  their  need.  On  the  other  hand, 
some  children  with  enuresis  present  problems,  for  even  if  they  awaken 

532 


with  a  need,  they  would  rather  wet  a  bed  and  return  to  sleep  than  get  up 
out  of  bed. 

The  sleep  iccords  obtained  from  children,  young  adults,  and  both  the 
normal  and  abnormal  older  people  show  piecisely  the  expected  pattern: 
Neither  the  youngsters  nor  young  adults  interrupt  their  sleep  to  go  to  the 
bathroom  during  the  night,  but  the  older  persons  rose  once  on  the  average 
and  the  senile  patients  slightly  more  often. 

Quite  apart  from  rising  to  visit  the  toilet  during  the  night,  adults  do 
awaken  from  sleep  intermittently.  The  normal  young  adult  will  awaken, 
albeit  briefly,  around  three  times  a  night.  The  awakenings  may  be  so  brief 
that  he  will  not  remember  them.  A  normal  older  person  will  awaken  five 
or  more  times  a  night.  However,  the  elderly  person  with  chronic  brain 
syndrome  will  awaken  eight  or  nine  times  a  night. 

Perhaps  some  of  these  differences  in  the  number  of  sleep  interruptions 
stein  from  the  different  arousal  speeds  of  the  young  and  old.  Youngsters 
are  typically  very  hard  to  awaken,  especially  during  the  first  half  of  the 
night  when  they  are  so  much  immersed  in  intense  delta  sleep.  Older 
people,  by  contrast,  have  less  slow-wave  sleep,  no  intense  stage  4,  and 
often  awaken  like  a  shot.  Children  are  virtually  somnambulistic  for  a  long 
time  after  awakening,  but  the  oldster  is  alert  at  once,  his  brain  having 
turned  off  sleep  like  a  faucet. 

The  insomnia  of  age  is  a  frequently  discussed  affliction.  Most  older 
people  would  appeal  to  have  no  stagc-4  sleep  and  less  deep  delta  sleep. 
They  are  more  easily  awakened  than  the  young.  Indeed,  they  do  take 
longer  to  fall  asleep,  awaken  more  often  during  the  night,  and  spend 
considerable  time  lying  in  bed  sleeplessly.  This  insomnia  has  been 
observed  by  Dr.  Fein  berg  and  his  associates,  by  Dr.  Anthony  Kales  and  his 
associates  at  UCLA,  and  by  Dr.  Wilse  B.  Webb  and  his  associates  at  the 
University  of  Florida,  From  childhood  on,  as  the  Webb  group  in  Florida 
has  shown,  there  is  a  decrease  in  delta  sleep  and  an  increase  in  awakenings. 
The  proportion  of  the  night  spent  without  sleep  increases  as  a  person 
grows  older,  but  as  Dr.  Fcinberg  and  his  colleagues  have  shown,  the  most 
egregious  symptoms  of  insomnia  are  found  in  patients  with  chronic  brain 
syndrome,  far  worse  than  in  normal  persons  of  the  same  advanced  age, 
The  children  studied  by  this  team  were  a  marked  contrast:  They  fell 
asleep  fast  and  slept  soundly  and  long.  The  5-year-olds  averaged  about  914 
hours,  while  the  10-year-olds  averaged  about  8!4  hours.  The  patients  with 
chronic  brain  syndrome  averaged  approximately  5  hours  of  sleep,  while 
the  young  adults  and  normal  older  people  slept  around  7  hours  a  night. 

Although  a  great  many  older  people  complain  of  the  insomnia  of  their 
waning  years,  they  are  not  accurate  judges  of  their  own  sleep.  It  has 
become  clear  in  the  laboratory,  where  the  EEC  record,  like  the  snore  of 
Hie  sleeper,  indicates  that  a  person  who  may  think  himself  awake  is  not 
actually  awake.  Dr.  Feinberg  has  said,  "A  lot  of  people  who  think  they're 
awake  arc  just  aware  of  their  mental  activity,  which  to  the  EEG  appears  to 
be  sleep.  Therefore  they  think  they  are  awake." 

People  do  not  even  estimate  very  accurately  the  amount  of  sleep  they 
obtain.  Dr.  Fcinberg  asked  his  elderly  volunteers  from  the  Fossils  Club 

533 


how  much  sleep  they  usually  got  at  home.  "There  was  zero  correlaii 
between  what  they  thought  and  the  amount  of  sleep  they  did  get,  Soi 
of  them  would  just  take  the  time  they  spent  in  bed  and  say  that  was  h< 
much  sleep  they  got.  Others  would  say  that  they  were  awake  most  oft 
night  but  that  they'd  simply  lie  there.  They  were  both  wrong." 

In  the  lifespan  picture  of  sleep,  certain  in  variances  are  striking.  0 
thing  that  seemed  not  to  change  between  ages  5  and  95  was  the  number 
REM  periods  during  a  night's  sleep.  Subjects  studied  by  Dr.  Feinberg, 
al.   consistently  showed  4-5  REM  periods.  The  children  would  sic* 
between  2  and  3  hours  before  their  first  REM  period;  the  young  adul 
took  about  an  average  of  70  minutes;  the  aged  people  about  58  minuti 
average;  and  the  chronic  brain  syndrome  patients  dropped  into  REM  slet 
relatively  rapidly— within  an  average  of  46  minutes.  The  amount  of  rap« 
eye-movement  sleep,  on  the  other  hand,  distributed   over  the   nigh 
showed  considerable  changes  with  age, 

Similarly  there  was  a  pronounced  reduction  in  delta  sleep-stages  3  an 
4- from  childhood  to  old  age.  Throughout  life,  whether  the  sleeper  i 
child  or  octogenarian,  it  appears  that  most  of  the  delta  or  slow-wave  slee 
occurs  mainly  at  the  beginning  of  the  night,  and  there  is  virtually  non 
during  the  last  period  of  non-REM  sleep.  At  that  time,  the  intcus 
oblivion  seems  to  have  run  its  course. 

When  Dr.  Wilse  B.  Webb  and  H.  W.  Agnew,  Jr.,  at  the  University  o 
Florida  invited  subjects  to  return  to  the  sleep  laboratory  for  a  nap  in  tin 
morning  after  a  full  night's  sleep,  they  found  that  there  was  virtually  nc 
slow-wave  sleep  and  no  stage  4  in  the  naps  of  these  young  adults.  REM 
sleep,  on  the  other  hand,  occurred  more  icadily  in  the  morning  than  it  did 
at  night.  The  distribution  of  stage  4  throughout  the  night  might  seem 
related  to  some  roughly  24-hour  rhythm  with  a  peak  coming  toward  the 
onset  of  sleep  rather  than  at  the  end-  But,  the  decrease  of  stage-4  sleep 
over  a  lifetime  is  striking. 

The  children  and  young  adults  exhibited  long  stretches  of  rhythmic 
delta  sleep  whereas  these  slow-wave  movements  were  always  interrupted 
by  fast  rhythms  in  the  EEGs  of  the  older  persons.  Dr.  Feinberg  has  sug- 
gested that  stage  3,  which  persists  in  lessened  form  into  old  age,  may  be  a 
less  intense  form  of  stage  4.  In  general,  the  older  people  showed  a  flat- 
tened EEC  tracing,  meaning  rhythms  of  lower  amplitude.  The  stage  3  of 
the  very  young,  the  adult,  and  the  aged  remained  much  the  same  in 
quantity  although  qualitative  differences  were  noted.  Even  in  the  senile, 
stage  3  activity  was  distributed  similarly  across  the  night,  as  if  controlled 
by  some  unchanging  and  inherent  periodicity. 


age—and  the  way  this  life  trend  can  be  read  in  the  EEC  patterns  of  sleep. 
Although  waking  EEGs  for  highly  abnormal  senile  patients  often  show 
little  deviation  from  the  normal,  sleep  EEGs  display  changes  so  gross  they 
cannot  be  missed.  The  senile  patient,  who  seems  to  represent  an  extreme 
of  the  normal  process  of  aging,  shows  a  sleep  pattern  that  is  also  an 
exaggeration  of  the  changes  observed  in  normal  elderly  people.  The 
investigators  began  to  ask  whether  certain  changes  in  the  sleep  record 
might  also  correlate  with  intellectual  functioning,  memory,  and  per- 
formance on  certain  tests. 

All  of  the  older  subjects  were  given  a  Wechsler  Adult  Intelligence  Scale, 
a  Wechsler  Memory  Scale,  and  another  test.  How  did  these  test  scores 
correlate  with  particular  changes  in  the  prevailing  stages  of  sleep?  Among 
the  senile  patients  it  was  possible  to  see  that  a  person  who  slept  longer,  lay 
awake  less  than  his  fellow  subjects,  but  had  more  REM  sleep,  also  gave  a 
higher  showing  on  the  intelligence  test.  The  normal  older  person,  who 
awakened  often  from  sleep  and  showed  a  decline  of  REM  sleep,  also  did 
correspondingly  less  well  on  the  psychometric  tests.  Since  some  senile 
persons  did  so  absurdly  on  the  psychometric  tests  that  it  was  impossible 
to  rate  their  performances,  an  observer  was  set  the  task  of  rating  these 
patients'  abilities  to  carry  out  the  simple  chores  of  life.  The  more  awaken- 
ings and  less  REM  sleep  they  showed,  the  less  intact  they  seemed  during 
these  daily  observations.  Awakenings  and  REM  sleep  appeared  to  be  two 
indicators  that  told  approximately  how  much  intellectual  impairment  to 
expect  in  an  individual.  Among  patients  with  chronic  brain  syndrome,  the 
sleep  shows  a  proportionately  greater  reduction  in  REM  sleep  than  in 
slow-wave  sleep. 

In  earlier  work,  Dr.  Feinberg  had  noted  that  lifetime  change  in  the  rate 
of  cerebral  metabolism  was  strikingly  similar  to  the  lifetime  curves  for 
total  sleep  time,  and  amount  of  REM  sleep.  Not  all  of  the  changes  in  sleep 
patterns  parallel  the  changing  cerebral  metabolic  rate.  However,  a  decline 
in  cerebral  metabolic  rate  commensurate  with  the  sleep-changes  exhibited 
in  senility  also  spells  intellectual  impairment  But  subtle  changes  in 
intellect  occur  earlier  in  the  nonnal  elderly  person.  These  changes,  which 
cannot  be  detected  by  measurement  of  overall  cerebral  metabolic  rate  or 
in  waking  EEC,  are  nevertheless  reflected  in  the  EEC  of  sleep. 

One  suggestive  parallel  in  the  age  graph  of  sleep  is  that  the  decline  of 
stage-4  sleep  occurs  as  a  person's  mental  agility  also  declines  and  as  it 
becomes  difficult  for  him  to  learn  psychomotor  skills.  The  investigators 
postulate  that  in  some  fashion  the  intensity  of  non-REM  sleep,  particular- 
ly as  represented  by  long  trains  of  high-synchronous  delta  waves,  repre- 
sents the  plasticity  of  the  individual's  learning  capacity.  Stage-4  sleep 
declines  as  this  plasticity  also  declines  in  age.  The  relation  between  the 
known  sleep  stages  and  an  individual's  cognitive  ability  will  be  the  focus 

of  further  study,  . 

The  relationship  between  cognitive  power  and  sleep  patterns  in  elderly 
people  may  evoke  a  profoundly  different  attitude  toward  some  of  the 
deteriorations  we  now  accept  as  the  usual  penalties  for  growing  old.  As 
one  researcher  has  conjectured,  it  maybe  that  the  hypenirousability  of  the 

535 


aged  and  their  repeated  awakenings  in  sleep  cause  a  reduction  in  REIv 
sleep  and  even  in  stage  4.  Loss  of  cognitive  power  may  in  some  way  bi 
related  to  an  inability  to  maintain  continuous  sleep.  As  other  researcher 
point  out,  "insomnia,"  disturbed  sleep  patterns,  and  intellectual  deteriora 
tion  are  not  found  in  all  old  people.  Perhaps  these  sleep  and  menta 
factors  also  have  a  very  strong  psychological  component  and  relate  to  tin 
sources  of  depression  in  so  many  old  people.  Perhaps  such  deterioratioi 
should  be  considered  pathological  even  in  its  most  usual  form,  instead  ol 
accepted  as  normal  for  a  given  age.  During  the  Elizabethan  era,  a  person 
had  normally  lost  most  of  his  teeth  and  might  be  considered  old  after  age 
25.  We  would  hardly  accept  this  as  normal  today. 

A  very  practical  outcome  of  these  studies  will  be  felt  in  the  treatment 
of  the  senile.  By  documenting  the  changed  sleep  patterns  which  occur  in 
old  age  and  chronic  brain  syndrome,  clinicians  are  acquiring  a  rational 
basis  for  treating  the  insomnia  of  the  elderly.  It  is  necessary  to  reveise 
these  age  changes  in  sleep  or  to  alter  the  sleep  schedule  so  that  the  effects 
are  minimized.  Dramatic  vagaries  shown  by  some  chronic  brain  syndrome 
subjects  as  they  awakened  from  REM  sleep  suggest  that  nocturnal 
delirium  and  wandering  may  result  from  a  confusion  of  dream  and  reality 
in  an  impaired  cerebrum,  or  perhaps  from  the  lingering  of  dreams  and 
other  REM  experiences  into  the  waking  state.  Drugs  that  reduce  the 
intensity  of  REM  processes  could  be  valuable  in  the  treating  of  these 
symptoms.  Since  nocturnal  disturbances  are  often  the  main  cause  for 
hospitalization,  such  treatment  might  allow  elderly  people  to  remain  in 
their  homes  and  communities  instead  of  spending  their  last  days  in  an 
institution.  As  Dr.  Feinberg  has  commented,  "The  changes  in  the  sleep 
electroencephalogram  which  occur  in  normal  old  age  as  well  as  in  chronic 
brain  syndrome,  and  the  correlation  of  these  changes  with  intellectual 
function,  suggest  that  the  EEC  of  sleep  may  provide  a  far  more  powerful 
diagnostic  tool  for  geriatric  psychiatry  than  any  which  has  been  hitherto 
available." 


Figure  1. -Total  sleep  time  as  a  function  of 
age,  In  this  and  subsequent  figures,  the 
crosses  represent  data  points,  the  dots 
represent  the  best-fitting  curve  chosen  ac- 
cording to  standard  statistical  techniques. 
The  number  of  subjects  contributing  to 
the  mean  for  each  data  point  is  as 
follows:  6  years,  4;  10  years,  4;  21  years, 
6;  30  years,  9;  69  years,  7;  84  years,  6.  A 
cubic  curve  provided  the  best  fit  for  the 
changes  in  total  sleep  time  with  age.  This 
measure  is  high  in  childhood,  declines  to 
a  plateau  which  is  maintained  during 
maturity,  and  then  shows  a  further 
decline  in  very  old  age.  The  slight  dip  and 
subsequent  rise  shown  by  the  theoretical 
curve  may  be  artifactual. 


JOE    IN  YEiflS 


536 


Figure  2.-Number  of  awakenings  during 
sleep  as  a  function  of  age.  Awakening 
here  refers  to  changes  in  the  EEC  and  was 
not  necessarily  accompanied  by  gross 
behavioral  arousal-  This  measure  is  low  in 
childhood  but  shows  a  steady,  linear 
increase  with  age  whidh  is  apparent 
during  maturity,  where  total  sleep  time  is 
constant. 


Figure  3. -Percent  of  time  in  bed  spent 
awake  as  a  function  of  age.  This  measure 
which  is  an  index  of  insomnia  remains 
low  throughout  life  and  then  shows  a 
sharp,  positively  accelerated  increase  after 
age  50  years.  The  data  of  Williams  et  al., 
on  subjects  in  the  sixth  decade  of  life, 
suggest  that  the  increase  starts  a  few  years 
later  than  shown  by  the  present  parabolic 
curve. 


ZO  40  60  BO  100 

BGE 


Figure  4. -Stage  4  EEG  as  a  function  of  age. 
This  measure  of  the  high-voltage,  slow- 
wave  EEG  of  sleep  shows  a  hyperbolic 
decline  with  age.  However,  appreciable 
change  is  manifested  during  maturity,  as 
may  be  seen  by  comparing  the  values  at 
age  20  and  age  50  years. 


Figure  5.— Stage  1  latency  as  a  function  of 
age.  This  measure  represents  the  total 
sleep  time  recorded  prior  to  the  onset  of 
the  first  period  of  emergent  stage  1  EEG 
of  REM  sleep.  It  shows  a  hyperbolic 
decline  with  age;  part  of  this  decline  is 
probably  related  to  the  declining  need  for 
stage  4  EEG,  half  of  which  precedes  the 
first  period  of  emergent  stage  1,  The 
curve  for  the  onset  of  another  indicator 
of  REM  sleep,  the  first  rapid-eye- 
movement,  is  essentially  the  same  as  that 
shown  here  for  emergent  stage  1  EEG, 


40  60 

AdE  {YEARS! 


537 


Figure  6. -Total  time  spent  nightly  in  stage  1 
EEC  (REM  sleep)  as  a  function  of  age. 
This  measure  shows  a  hyperbolic  decline 
with  little  appreciable  change  during 
maturity.  The  curve  for  amount  of 
rap  id-e  ye  -movement  activity  is  essentially 
the  same  as  that  for  emergent  stage  1 
EEC. 


40  60 

AGE  IN  VEflRg 


Figure  7  .—Cerebral  oxygen  uptake  as  a 
function  of  age  These  data  are  taken 
from  various  sources  in  the  literature  and 
are  based  on  the  Kety-Schmidt  method 
for  measuring  oxygen  uptake  (CMRO2). 
This  variable  shows  a  hyperbolic  decline 
with  age  with  little  change  during  the 
mature  years.  Since  the  data  did  not  fall 
into  well-defined  age  subgroups,  individ- 
ual values,  rather  than  mean  values  for 
the  different  ages,  are  shown  here.  This 
largely  accounts  for  the  apparent 
increased  scatter  shown, 


Figure  8. -Cortical  cell  density  and  brain 
weight  as  a  function  of  age.  The  decline 
in  cell  density  during  maturity  is  con- 
siderable, whereas  that  for  brain  weight 
is,  in  percentage  termSj  less  marked. 


Figure  9.-Wechsler  Adult  Intelligence  Scale 
(WAIS)  verbal  and  performance  scores  as 
a  function  of  age.  Both  measures  show  a 
parabolic  decline.  However,  the  verbal 
scores  show  little  change  during  maturity 
whereas  the  performance  scores  show  a 
reduction  of  about  30  percent  between 
age  20  and  age  50  years. 


Research  Grant:  MH  10927 
Date  of  Interview:  Apr.  26,  1967 


tt     SO 


co«ric«,  ciu.  tiNSilr 


'  -.7- 


Pirlormonce 
** 


To" 


"75 


AGE  {YEARS) 


538 


Irwestigutors: 

Mark  R.  Rosenzweig,  Ph.D.* 

Edward  L  Bennett,  Ph.D. 

David  Krech,  Ph.D. 

Marian  C.  Diamond,  Ph.D. 

University  of  California 
Berkeley,  Calif. 

Propnrod  by: 

Gay  Luco 


In  the  last  dozen  years  a  Berkeley  team  of  psychologists,  biochemists, 
ami  anatomists  has  demonstrated  that  learning  niters  chemical  activity  and 
increases  cell  growth  in  the  cerebral  cortex— changes  that  arc  correlated 
with  an  enhanced  problem-solving  ability.  Thus,  experiments  with  young 
and  with  adult  animals  show  that  mental  activity  produces  detectable 
brain  growth  and  that  specific  kinds  of  activity  may  develop  particular 
regions  of  the  brain.  Although  there  may  be  limits  to  an  animal's  respon- 
siveness to  its  environment,  a  stimulating  environment  appears  to 
maximize  the  rate  of  learning. 

Using  rats  as  experimental  subjects,  the  scientists  maximized  environ- 
mental contrast  by  segregating  littermates  into  two  extreme  situations. 
Some  were  reared  in  isolation  in  small  barren  cages  in  a  dim,  quiet  room. 
Their  brothers  matured  in  groups  of  10—12  in  large  complex  ciigcs 
supplied  with  toys,  exercise  wheels,  and  other  diversions,  as  well  as  daily 
training  on  laboratory  apparatus.  These  enriched  rats  erred  less  on 
problem-solving  tests,  and  postmortem  analyses  showed  thai  their  brains 
differed  from  the  brains  of  their  impoverished  siblings.  The  cerebral 
cortex  had  grown  larger  in  relation  to  the  rest  of  the  brain.  H  continued 
more  glial  cells  and  showed  greater  activity  in  a  particular  biochemical 
system. 

Animals  exposed  to  the  differential  environments  after  adulthood 
exhibited  much  the  same  differences  in  brain  growth. 

A  variety  of  genetic  studies  indicated  that  the  proportion  of  the  brain 
occupied  by  cerebral  cortex  and  also  certain  chemical  attributes  of  the 
brains  of  enriched  rats  were  characteristic  of  "bright"  strains,  while  "dull" 
strains  more  nearly  resembled  impoverished  animals. 


*Now    at    the   Department   of  Psychology,  Washington   University,    St.    Louis, 
Missouri. 

539 


The  experimenters  are  now  testing  for  the  role  of  formal  train 
brain  development.  Will  a  rat  reared  alone  in  an  empty  cage  show 
growth  and  an  improved  problem-solving  ability  after  intensive  daity 
ing  on  an  automated  teaching  apparatus?  Can  formal  training  be  us 
compensate  for  lack  of  environmental  complexity  and  stimulation? 

The  grantees  have  begun  to  push  this  field  forward  during  a  time 
it  was  believed  impossible  to  detect  physical  effects  of  learning  j 
brain.  Their  data  may  help  to  explain  many  baffling  statistical  ob 
tions  about  human  beings,  among  them  the  role  of  environment  in 
ligence,  the  accomplishments  of  people  with  median  childhood  IQ 
the  curious  incidence  of  eminence  among  first-born  children.  The  la 
tory  has  seen  some  evidence  that  specific  kinds  of  environn 
manipulation  enhance  growth  in  specific  brain  areas,  suggesting  thi 
may  learn  to  provide  rehabilitation  training  for  peisons  with  brain  i 
or  sensory  deficiencies—giving  exercise  to  develop  brain  regions, 

Since  the  brain  is  measurably  modified  by  use,  in  a  manner  that  c 
correlated  with  adaptiveness  and  learning  capacity,  the  laboral 
findings  have  strong  implications  for  the  education  of  children.  Ho\\ 
the  extreme  impoverishment  of  the  experimental  rat  should  IK 
translated  as  if  it  were  synonymous  with  cultural  deprivation,  A  vat 
of  experience  is  rare  among  human  infants,  unless  they  are  doaf-t 
However,  there  is  no  escaping  the  conclusion  that  we  literally  mod  if1 
brains  according  to  the  use  we  make  of  them.  Through  research  prog 
like  this  one,  we  may  gain  the  impetus  to  deliberately  improve  the  ei 
tion  and  mental  equipment  of  our  young,  for  there  is  some  potency  i 
knowledge  that  we  are  measurably  altering  their  brains. 


Background 

Scientists  in  the  past  have  felt  that  use  of  the  brain  might  affect  its 
size  and  composition.  Charles  Darwin,  for  instance,  observed 
domestic  rabbits  had  smaller  brains  than  wild  rabbits.  He  surmised 
the  domestic  rabbit  had  been  so  confined  and  protected  for  many  gci 
tions  that  it  exercised  its  intellect  and  instincts  very  little.  He  implied 
lack  of  mental  activity  and  stimulation  had  caused  brain  atrophy,  h 
late  19th  century,  the  autopsy  of  Laura  Bridgman,  a  deaf-blind  n 
showed  deficient  development  in  visual,  auditory,  and  speech  regioi 
the  brain.  Recently,  animals  reared  in  darkness  have  been  given  ca> 
post  mortem  examinations.  They  showed  shrinkage  in  the  visual  pai 
the  cortex. 

The  notion  that  thinking  might  cause  the  brain  to  grow  was  suggc 
during  the  18th  and  19th  centuries.  A  German  phrenologist,  J 
Spurzheim,  believed  that  the  brain  grew  by  exercise,  but  he  had  no  m 
of  demonstrating  the  phenomenon.  Even  in  his  day  the  brain  waskn 
to  be  relatively  stable,  and  there  was  no  equipment  for  testing  sti 
changes.  Ideally,  it  was  proposed,  one  should  examine  the  same  pc 
after  a  long  period  of  intense  brainwork  and  after  long  period! 

540 


desuetude;  or  perhaps  compare  twins,  after  keeping  one  isolated  and  idle, 
while  the  other  lived  a  rich  and  mentally  active  life.  This  experiment 
was  hardly  suited  for  human  beings.  It  had  to  await  the  modern  animal 
laboratory,  with  its  refined  instruments. 

Today,  people  are  aware  that  the  brain  is  the  physical  machinery  of  the 
mind  and  that  it  can  be  affected  by  physical  means-by  drugs,  electrical 
stimulation,  etc.  The  rate  of  brain  activity,  memory,  and  emotions  are 
related    to   the   actions  of  chemicals  in  the  brain;  and  when  we  alter 
behavior,  memory,  and  emotions  by  drugs,  we  thus  change  many  factors 
in  what  we  consider  to  be  intelligence.  In  the  last  century  intelligence  was 
presumptively    equated   with  brain  size.  Many  attempts  were  made  to 
compare  the  brain  size  of  men  with  distinguished  intellects  and  with  those 
of  inferior  intelligence.  Gross  brain  sizes  were  found  to  be  inconsistent, 
however,  and  an  idiot  can  have  a  larger  brain  than  Einstein.  Thus,  this 
unsuccessful  approach  was  abandoned.  The  notion  of  anatomical  change 
and  actual  increase  in  brain  weight  as  a  result  of  cerebration  became  so 
widely   debunked  that  the  grantees,  themselves,  ignored  this  possibility 
until  they  were  well  along  in  their  research  program. 

In  J  953  they  began  search  for  some  of  the  physiochemical  correlates  of 
learning,  an  approach  that  required  considerable  courage  in  the  skeptical 
atmosphere  of  that  time.  They  had  started  by  observing  the  behavior  of 
hungry    laboratory  rats  who  were  placed  in  a  problem-solving  test  box 
invented  by  Dr.  Krech.  The  Krech  Hypothesis  Apparatus  Box  is  a  simple 
piece  of  equipment  that  has  been  widely  used  for  a  number  of  purposes. 
This  is  a  long  box,  divided  into  four  successive  chambers.  In  each  chamber 
the  hungry  animal  chooses  between  a  left  and  a  right  alley.  He  must  make 
four  correct  choices  to  reach  the  food  compartment  at  the  end.  In  each 
vestibule  he  may  face  a  darkened  alley  on  one  side  and  a  well-lighted  alley 
on  the  other.  Which  side  has  the  open  door?  In  one  problem  the  light  may 
be  a  clue  to  the  correct  side.  That  is,  the  animal  may  find  heisiewarded 
when   he  chooses  the  lighted  alley,  whichever  side  this  happens  to  be. 
When  he  almost  invariably  chooses  this  avenue  and  quickly  runs  to  his 
food  reward  without  digressions,  he  suddenly  finds  that  light  is  no  longer 
the  rules  of  the  game.  He  must  now  learn  to  choose  the  dark  avenue.  At 
first  he  has  learned  a  simple  discrimination,  and  then  he  must  learn  how  to 
reverse  his  discrimination.  From  the  animal's  point  of  view  it  may  seem 
that,  as  soon  as  he  adopts  the  profitable  habit,  the  rules  are  changed.  His 
adaptability— and  presumably  his  survival  in  the  outside  world-depend  in 
part  upon  his  ability  in  this  reversal  discrimination  learning.  Thus,  in  many 
experiments,  reversal  discrimination  provides  a  crucial  performance  test. 
In  the  laboratory's  original  studies,  the  hungry  rats  were  bucking  an 
insoluble  problem  in  the  Krech  apparatus,  The  choices  were  prearranged 
so   that   there   was  no  correct  solution.  It  was  observed,  however,  that 
individual  rats  had  interesting  choice  patterns.  They  acted  as  if  following 
some  hypothesis  about  the  rules  of  the  game.  When,  later,  the  situation 
was  made  progressively  more  solvable-for  instance  when  light  became  the 
correct   cue  70  percent  of  the  time-an  animal  might  still  prefer  to  go 
down   the  left  side,  much  of  the  time,  disregarding  the  light  cue.  Some 


animals  acted  as  If  the  visual  cues  of  light  and  dark  were  irrelevant 
would  make  a  spatial  choice,  running  down  the  right  avenue  each  1 
Others  seemed  to  fixate  on  visual  cues,  ignoring  the  possibility  that  I 
did  not  indicate  the  correct  exit.  Because  the  hypothesis  box  rev* 
these  individual  differences  in  the  rats'  ability  to  restrict  their  attentic 
the  pertinent  cues,  it  seemed  to  provide  an  excellent  test  of  prob 
solving  ability. 

When  lats  were  allowed  to  run  through  the  alley  as  they  wished,  nc 
being  uniformly  correct,  some  animals  followed  the  light  and  otto 
spatial  pattern.  The  population  seemed  to  divide  into  rats  with  a  pr 
ence  for  visual  cues  and  rats  with  a  proclivity  for  spatial  cues.  On  rev 
tests  in  the  alley,  the  "spatial"  rats  proved  better  at  adapting  to  the 
rules.  Thus,  in  a  sense,  they  seemed  brighter.  The  experimei 
postulated  that  these  rats  might  have  brains  that  were  richer  ii 
important  enzyme -related  in  its  quantity  to  a  chemical  that  I 
transmit  excitement  from  one  brain  cell  to  the  next.  At  the  time  it  see 
plausible  that  quick-learning  animals  might  have  an  abundant  suppl 
this  chemical  so  important  in  nervous  activity.  The  experimei 
expected  to  find  that  rat  strains  which  demonstrated  superior  lear 
ability  would  show  more  of  a  particular  brain  enzyme,  but  the  dun 
animals  actually  showed  more.  In  testing  more  strains  of  rats,  they  bi 
to  wonder  if  the  neuro transmitter  and  enzyme  were  genetically 
dependent,  or  indeed  whether  they  were  seeking  the  right  chemica 
correlate  with  behavior. 


Biochemical  Indices:  The  Acetylcholine  System 

Brain  activity  is  both  electrical  and  chemical.  When  a  person  think 
takes  an  action,  neurons  pulse  out  rhythmic  coded  messages.  Each  b; 
cell  influences  neighboring  cells  biochemically.  As  it  fires,  it  relcasi 
chemical  at  the  infinitesimal  gaps  or  synapses  between  itself  and  surroi 
ing  cells.  This  chemical,  known  as  a  neuro  transmitter,  bridges  the  gar 
can  change  the  excitability  of  the  neighboring  cells  so  that  they 
readied  to  fire.  The  safeguard  that  prevents  cells  from  conlinu 
excitability  and  exhaustion  is  an  inactivating  chemical.  Each  lini 
transmitter  is  released,  it  is  quickly  neutralized  by  an  enzyme. 

One  of  the  first  neurotransmitters  to  be  studied  was  acetylcholme, 
released  at  nerve  endings  throughout  the  body,  in  the  regulation 
muscular  contraction  and  relaxation,  throughout  the  periphery  and  in 
heart  and  other  organs.  Acetylcholine  is  released  at  a  synapse^  t 
hydrolyzed  by  the  enzymes  acetylcholinesterase  (AChFi)  , 
cholinesterase  (ChE)— whereupon  the  excitability  of  junction  po 
subsides  and  they  return  to  their  resting  state.  Acetylcholine  and  its 
activating  enzymes  are  found  throughout  the  brain. 

A  high  rate  of  brain  activity,  as  in  learning,  might  require  a  rapid : 
constant  output  of  the  transmitter  and  the  enzymes  that  destroy  it. 

542 


In  the  mid-1950's  the  grantees  began  to  explore  the  possibility  that 
bright  animals  had  brains  richer  in  acetylcholine  and  that  an  environment 
enforcing  a  great  deal  of  learning  might  change  the  output  of  this 
transmitter  in  the  brain.  It  was  then  technically  difficult  to  obtain  precise 
evaluations  of  acetylcholine  in  brain  tissue,  and  so  they  worked  by 
indirection.  Presumably  the  activity  of  the  destroying  enzymes  was  related 
to  acetylcholine  concentiation. 

Because  the  enzymes  AChE  and  ChE  would  remain  relatively  stable  in 
frozen  brain  tissue,  it  was  possible  to  measure  their  speed  of  activity  in  a 
relatively  direct  way.  A  sample  of  homogenized  brain  tissue  was  injected 
into  a  fluid  containing  a  great  deal  of  acetythiolcholine,  which  the  enzyme 
breaks  down  at  the  same  rate  as  acetylcholine.  When  light  was  passed 
through  the  fluid,  and  an  indicator  was  added,  its  color  would  change  as 
the  enzymes  acted,  producing  a  yellow  compound;  the  color  change 
could  be  measured,  and  told  how  fast  the  enzymes  were  destroying  the 
transmitter. 

Originally,  acetylcholinesterase  was  the  better  known  enzyme.  Its 
activity  was  first  used  in  an  attempt  to  find  a  chemical  index  of  brain 
efficiency.  However,  as  many  strains  of  rat  were  tested  on  problem-solving 
apparatus,  the  poorest  performers  often  showed  the  highest  AChE 
activity.  Yet  some  strains,  with  excellent  learning  records,  showed  high 
ChE  activity, 

During  the  late  1 950's  the  experimenters  were  able  to  directly  evaluate 
the  relation  between  the  transmitter,  acetylcholine,  and  the  activity  of  the 
enzyme  AChE  in  two  rat  strains.  Although  one  strain  had  significantly 
greater  amounts  of  acetylcholine,  the  AChE  enzyme  was  not  propor- 
tionately more  active.  They  found  that  learning  experience  or  that 
"brightness"  in  an  animal  was  indicated  in  the  ratio  between  ChE  and 
AChE  activity  in  different  portions  of  the  brain.  AChE  activity  bore  moie 
directly  upon  the  output  of  the  transmitter,  and  the  role  of  the  other  less 
specific  enzyme  ChE  was  not  clear. 

By  selective  breeding  the  researchers  acquired  strains  of  rats  whose 
brains  differed  in  acetylcholine  concentration,  or  in  its  enzyme,  AChE.  On 
successive  tests  there  has  been  considerable  uncertainty  about  the  relative 
intelligence  of  different  rat  strains  and  their  brain  characteristics.  Students 
in  the  laboratory,  and  consultants,  have  begun  to  amass  information  about 
the  different  rat  strains.  For  example,  a  strain  with  high  brain  acetyl- 
choline  would  react  differently  to  electric  shock  than  another  strain.  This 
fact  has  some  importance  to  other  researchers,  especially  those  who  might 
employ  these  animals  in  experiments  using  shock  punishment.  The  genetic 
studies  have  continued  to  be  part  of  the  laboratory  program.  The  team  has 
attempted  to  control  as  much  as  possible  for  heredity,  as  it  has  begun  to 
track  the  way  in  which  environment  might  change  brain  biochemistry. 

Experience  and  Learning  Capacity 

During  the   1950's  a  number  of  laboratory  e 
early    experience  would   make  an  enormous 


ability  to  cope  with  learning  problems.  In  one  laboratory,  animals  rea 
in  isolation  with  a  minimum  of  handling  turned  out  to  have  differ 
adrenal  responses  and  skittery  reactions  to  tests  or  stresses,  in  comparii 
with  littermates  raised  in  groups  with  considerable  stimulation.  Isola 
animals  performed  poozly  in  comparison  with  handled  littermates,  I 
were  their  nervous  systems  different? 


The  Enriched  and  the  Impoverished 

Over  almost  a  decade  the  grantees  have  employed  a  method  t!i 
maximized  the  contrast  between  their  animals,  prior  to  testing  and  bra 
assay.  Littermates  were  segregated  into  extreme  environments.  In  ea 
case  lats  of  a  particular  age,  strain,  and  sex  were  used.  During  subseque 
brain  assays,  the  rats  were  identified  only  by  codes  so  that  the  e 
perimenters  would  not  know  from  which  group  they  came.  This  proc 
dure,  initially  used  with  weanling  rats,  has  since  been  used  with  adults  at 
othci  species. 

In  general  the  infant  rats  were  segregated  when  they  were  25  days  oli 
At  this  point,  some  pups  would  land  in  barren  individual  cages  in  a  din 
quiet  room  unable  to  see  or  touch  othei  animals,  being  handled  on! 
during  biweekly  weighing.  Their  littermates,  in  groups  of  10-12,  foun 
themselves  surrounded  by  toys  and  were  handled  each  day  as  they  wei 
set  to  play  in  a  square  box  with  wooden  barriers  or  mazes.  In  man 
experiments,  the  rats  lived  their  extraordinarily  different  lives  for  81 
days-well  into  maturity.  At  this  point  they  were  tested  and  analyzed.  Ii 
some  experiments  the  environmental  exposure  lasted  only  a  month  or  51 
days. 

By  1966,  the  laboratory  had  amassed  data  on  well  over  200  pairs  01 
littermates.  The  evidence  was  consistent.  Enriched  rats  were  supeiior  k 
their  impoverished  brothers  on  reversal  discrimination  learning.  Alonj 
with  their  greater  problem-solving  abilities,  they  had  a  far  different  brain. 
The  cerebral  cortex,  often  colloquially  termed  the  thinking  brain,  had 
grown  larger  relative  to  the  rest  of  the  brain,  and  they  showed  a  different 
balance  in  the  activities  of  the  enzymes  cholinesterasc  (ChE)  and  acelyl- 
cholhtesterase  (AChE). 


Brain  Growth 

The  research  team  had  already  found  consistent  biochemical  changes 
before  they  began  to  look  at  changes  in  brain  weight.  As  Dr,  Rosenzweig 
has  explained: 

Brain  anatomy  was  disregarded  since  we  had  inherited  from  our 
predecessors  the  dogma  of  absolute  stability  of  brain  weight.  Fortunately 
we  had  to  record  the  weights  of  our  brain  samples  in  order  to  measure 
chemical  activity  per  unit  of  tissue  weight.  After  about  2  years  of 
contemplating  the  chemical  effects,  it  finally  dawned  on  us  that  the 

544 


weights  of  the  brain  samples  were  also  being  altered  by  the  environmental 
manipulations. 

Had  the  researchers  been  comparing  the  weights  of  whole  brains  they 
would  have  noticed  no  diffeiences.  Indeed,  from  outside,  one  might  have 
judged  that  the  impoverished  animals  showed  the  advantage.  When 
phrenologists  practiced  their  art  of  measuring  intelligence  and  personality 
by  comparing  proportions  of  the  skull,  there  had  been  some  expectation 
that  a  large  skull  encompassed  a  larger  brain.  The  large  forehead  and 
cranium  which  appear  to  encase  a  more  sizable  Intelligence  turns  out  to  be 
a  poor  index  of  intelligence.  The  experimenters  compared  the  inner 
cranial  capacity  of  some  of  their  enriched  and  impoverished  animals  and 
found  that  the  two  groups  did  not  differ.  By  making  meticulous  rubber 
casts  of  the  skulls  and  measuring  facial  bones,  after  weighing  the  brains, 
they  saw  that  the  larger  facial  bones  were  those  of  the  impoverished 
animals  and  were  related  to  body  size  rather  than  to  brain  development. 
The  impoverished  and  inactive  creatures  were  both  heavier  and  larger  than 
their  littermates. 

Comparisons  of  whole-brain  weights  would  not  have  defined  the 
anatomical  differences  between  the  enriched  and  impoverished  rats. 
However,  in  preparing  brains  for  chemical  analysis,  the  experimenters  have 
been  dissecting  the  cerebral  cortex  into  four  sections  and  treating  these 
sepaiately  from  all  the  rest  of  the  brain  beneath.  They  had  reasoned, 
plausibly,  that  the  cerebral  cortex  should  be  the  part  of  the  brain  to  show 
(he  most  changes  as  a  result  of  learning. 

Indeed,  as  they  soon  saw,  the  enriched  animals  had  a  heavier  cortex 
than  did  their  impoverished  brothers.  When  141  pairs  of  litteimates  of  a 
particular  genetic  strain  were  compared,  the  enriched  animal  had  a  cortex 
(hat  was,  on  the  average,  4  percent  heavier  than  that  of  his  impoverished 
brother.  The  impoverished  animals  showed  their  growth -a  very  slight 
one— in  the  more  primitive  subcortex.  The  delicacy  of  these  procedures  is 
difficult  to  convey.  Altogether,  a  rat  brain  is  not  much  larger  than  a 
healthy  Brazil  nut.  Still,  if  the  percentage  differences  do  not  sound 
enormous,  the  brain  is  generally  so  stable  that  these  signs  of  growth, 
relative  to  experience,  are  impressive. 

As  the  experiments  were  varied,  and  as  the  scientists  looked  closely  at 
the  several  regions  of  the  cortex,  they  found  that  certain  portions  of  the 
cortex  were  changing  more  than  others.  The  occipital  region,  the  visual 
area  located  at  the  back  of  the  brain,  showed  the  most  growth.  The 
enriched  animal  had  an  occipital  cortex  that  was  6  percent  heavier  than 
that  of  his  impoverished  littermate,  Other  regions  showed  less  difference. 
A  number  of  experiments  were  conducted  in  order  to  see  whether  specific 
cortical  areas  could  be  caused  to  grow  through  the  use  of  specific  training 
and  environment. 

Animals  reared  in  darkness  have  been  found  to  show  a  lack  ol  growth  m 
the  visual  cortex.  When  the  researchers  segregated  blinded  animals  into 
extreme  environments,  they  found  that  the  complex  environment  and 
training  of  the  enriched  animals  did  compensate  somewhat  for  lack  ol 
sight.  Although  the  blind  animals  showed  enzymatic  differences  that 

545 


distinguished  them  fiom  the  sighted  animals,  they  too  responded 
impoverishment  or  stimulating  surroundings  in  very  much  the  s;i 
manner.  Signs  of  growth  in  the  visual  cortex  of  enriched  blind  i 
suggested  that  it  must  participate  in  nonvisual  functions.  Thus,  !>l 
animals  benefited  from  the  envhonmental  stimulation. 

AH  of  the  enriched  rats  have  shown  a  thickening  in  the  gray  outer  h; 
of  the  brain  and  also  in  a  subcortical  region  that  has  been  associated  \v 
memory  functions  and  refinement  of  emotion-the  hippocampus.  Sttnui 
tion,  enforcing  active  use  of  the  brain,  had  caused  palpable  growth  in  t 
cerebral  cortex  of  the  enriched  animal.  Just  as  specific  sensory  dcprivult 
such  as  blindness  or  deafness  would  cause  a  deficiency  in  Lho  relal 
cortical  region,  particular  experience  seemed  to  produce  growth  in 
relevant  cortical  area. 


Chemical  Changes 

Originally,  the  investigators  had  expected  to  find  that  their 
animals  showed  increases  in  the  specific  activity  of  the  enzyme  (AC '111: 
indicating  greater  concentrations  of  the  ncurotransmitter  acelyleholin 
Indeed,  the  enriched  cieatures  did  have  a  cortex  whose  tissue  showed  mm 
enzyme  activity  than  that  of  their  impoverished  brethren.  On  the  oUu 
hand,  the  AChE  activity  was  a  smaller  percent  per  unit  weight  In  Eh 
cortex  of  the  enriched.  The  growth  of  the  cortex  outdistanced  th 
increase  in  this  particular  enzymatic  activity.  However,  it  showed  , 
proportional  increase  in  the  subcortex.  If  AChE  activity  declined  in  tli> 
cortex,  it  increased  in  the  subcortex. 

In  order  to  pinpoint  the  biochemical  change  in  the  cortex,  the  re 
searchers  began  looking  at  the  proportions  of  the  specific  enzyme  AClil 
to  the  nonspecific  enzyme  ChE  in  the  cortex  and  rest  of  the  bmin:  in  Ifu 
brains  of  the  enriched  animal,  there  was  more  cortical  ChE  relative  to 
AChE.  The  relative  proportions  of  these  two  enzymes  thus  inclieiikul  (hi 
difference  between  the  enriched  and  impoverished  animals. 

When  the  scientists  looked  at  these  enzyme  ratios  in  specific  portions  in 
the  brain,  they  found  especially  striking  differences  between  ihcir 
enriched  and  impoverished  animals  in  the  visual  cortex. 

They  were  more  than  a  little  curious  to  find  that  the  cholincslcNM; 
activity  was  exceeding  that  of  the  more  specific  enzyme  in  their  enncliol 
animals.  Cholinesterase  is  known  to  be  concentrated  in  the  gliul-  or  mm- 
nerve  cells-of  the  brain.  As  they  searched  to  see  precisely  whfll  livwtf 
growth  accounted  for  the  heavier  cortex  of  the  enriched  animals,  (hey 
suspected  a  proliferation  of  these  important  small  cells. 


A  Multiplication  of  Brain  Cells 

The  experimenters  measured  the  diameter  of  capillaries  in  the 
and  found  them  to  have  grown  larger  in  the  enriched  animals  than  in 

546 


impoverished  animals.  But  this  enlargement  in  the  brain's  blood  supply 
network  did  not  explain  the  amount  of  cortical  growth.  They  subsequent- 
ly made  a  cell  count  in  the  cortex— the  region  that  showed  most  growth  in 
the  enriched  animal.  Frozen  tissue,  stained  and  cut  paper  thin,  was  made 
into  slides,  and  sizable  photographic  enlargements  weie  made  of  each 
slide.  Now  began  the  count  of  individual  cells. 

Two  anatomists  would  make  sepaiate  counts.  A  technician  using 
colored  pencils  would  mark  the  location  of  each  glial  and  neuronal  cell  on 
a  sheet  of  plastic  placed  on  the  photographic  enlargement  of  a  brain 
section.  The  same  process  would  be  repeated  by  a  second  technician. 
When  the  two  sheets  were  superimposed,  the  discrepancies  could  be  seen 
and  discussed,  and  finally  all  the  brain  cells  would  be  classified  and 
counted. 

A  comparison  was  made  of  the  visual  cortex  of  17  pairs  of  littermates. 
The  enriched  animals  had  more  glial  cells  than  did  the  impoverished,  a 
higher  ratio  of  glial  cells  to  neurons.  Glia  are  not  well  understood,  but 
tliey  are  believed  to  nourish  the  neuron  and  to  modulate  the  brain 
activity  by  altering  the  excitability  of  the  neurons.  Environmental 
complexity  and  stimulation  had  caused  a  proliferation  of  these  cells,  thus 
perhaps  enhancing  the  efficiency  of  the  neuronal  activity. 

The  proliferation  of  the  glia  was  confirmed  at  MIT  by  Drs.  Joseph 
Altman  and  Gopal  D.  Das.  Using  the  same  extreme  environments,  they 
analyzed  their  animals'  brains  by  a  different  method.  Each  animal  was 
injected  with  a  radioactive  substance  that  is  used  in  formation  of  new 
cells.  This  substance  acts  as  a  label.  Wherever  it  was  absorbed  by  a  brain 
cell,  there  would  be  slight  emissions  of  nuclear  particles  which  affect  a 
photographic  emulsion  in  the  manner  that  light  darkens  the  silver  grains  of 
any  ordinary  photographic  emulsion.  When  the  experiment  was  concluded 
and  brain  sections  were  dipped  in  emulsion,  photomicrographs  showed  a 
significant  increase  in  glial  cells  in  the  cortex  and  neocortex  of  the 
enriched  animals.  The  density  of  these  labeled  cells-showing  up  as  dark 
spots  on  the  film-can  be  measured  automatically. 

Drs.  Altman  and  Das  have  shown  that  in  the  rat  neural  cells  also 
multiply  after  birth.  It  has  long  been  supposed  that  an  animal  possesses  its 
entire  lifetime  supply  of  neurons  at  birth.  By  injecting  a  radioactive 
component  of  the  genetic  molecule,  DNA,  they  have  observed  that  the 
radioactive  substance  was  incorporated  in  the  formation  of  many  new 
brain  cells  in  adult  animals.  These  cells-so  small  they  are  known  as 
microneurons -appear  to  migrate  as  they  differentiate,  moving  into  new 
brain  areas.  It  now  seems  possible  that  some  of  these  small  neural  cells 
have  been  mistaken  for  glia.  The  MIT  team  has  opened  a  new  question  in 
the  role  of  experience  in  shaping  the  brain.  Will  older  adults  show  an 
increase  in  these  microneurons?  Do  they  multiply  in  response  to  the 
exercise  of  the  brain? 

Even  if  glial  cells  are  the  only  population  to  increase  in  the  brain  as  a 
result  of  environmental  challenge,  they  may  enhance  brain  efficiency  in  a 
number  of  ways.  Glia  are  important  in  the  fatty  white  matter  that 
sheathes  nerves.  They  surround  the  dendritic  tentacles  that  interconnect 

547 


each  neuron  with  multitudes  of  others.  Perhaps  their  multiplict 
permits  the  sprouting  of  new  dendrites,  new  contacts  from  one  ncurc 
others.  Perhaps  their  proliferation  allows  greater  nourishment  of  ni 
cells,  or  more  refined  modulation  of  neuron  excitability.  All  that  c.i 
said  at  present  is  that  environmental  manipulation  adds  to  the  lissu 
the  ceiebral  cortex  in  part  by  the  multiplication  of  glia  and  that  ani 
exhibiting  this  brain  growth  excel  on  problem-solving  tests. 

How  Do  the  Different  Environmental  Factors  Cause  Brain  Changes? 

Clearly,  when  an  animal's  entire  environment  is  manipulated  in 
laboratory,  it  is  difficult  to  pinpoint  the  particular  factois  responsible 
this  change.  Isolation,  for  instance,  has  a  potent  effect  upon  man 
beast.  People  who  have  undergone  tests  in  silent  chambers,  or  in  tanfe 
water  at  about  blood  temperature,  have  noted  that  the  sensory  ilcpi 
tion  began  to  evoke  odd  psychological  experiences.  Some  people  quit 
leave  such  an  experiment,  while  others  have  suffered  little.  Some  stinin 
rats,  reared  in  isolation,  have  become  increasingly  aggressive  and  difft 
to  handle  in  the  laboratory.  They  suffer  enlarged  adrenal  glands, ; 
irritations,  and  other  symptoms  known  as  isolation  stress.  None  of 
Berkeley    animals  showed   any  of  these   signs.   Nonetheless,   the  h 
inevitably   wanted   to  ascertain  what  role  isolation  contributed  to 
contrasting  brain  and  behavior  effects  of  the  impovereshed  and  ciiiic! 
rats. 

Social  controls. -Some  experiments  were  run  with  a  third  group 
littermates,  reared  neither  in  isolation  nor  in  a  complex  envJronnu 
These  rats  grew  up  in  the  ordinary  animal  laboratory  cages  in  gioups 
three.  The  only  sign  that  the  social  life  was  better  than  isolation  i 
detected  in  the  visual  cortex,  where  growth  exceeded  that  of 
impoverished  littermates.  On  other  counts,  the  brains  more  clos 
resembled  those  of  the  impoverished  animals  than  of  the  enriched. 

Extreme  isolation.— In  recent  studies  a  condition  has  been  creal 
approaching  a  sensory  vacuum.  Some  littermates  have  lived  where  Itglu 
dim  and  temperature  constant.  The  rat  cannot  see  outside  the  ciigc,  n 
even  having  contact  with  the  experimenter  as  he  changes  food  and  wal 
The  only  change  is  the  alternation  of  light  and  darkness  every  1 2  hours, 

As  might  have  been  expected,  rats  who  spent  80  days  in  this  envirt 
ment  proved  to  be  extremely  different  from  their  littcrmales  iti  I 
enriched  condition.  The  differences  in  cortical  weight  and  enzyme  flctbi 
were  markedly  enhanced.  Indeed,  these  extremely  isolated  unit  ii 
poverished  animals  were  different  from  rats  merely  raised  in  the  origin 
loneliness  of  unadorned  cages. 

Isolated  pairs.-W&$  social  isolation,  or  lack  of  a  stimulating  cnvira 
ment,  the  overriding  factor  in  the  lack  of  brain  development  in  I! 
extreme  isolates?  Pairs  of  rats  were  now  placed  in  the  extreme  isoliitio 
each  one  from  a  different  litter.  Post  mortem  analyses  of  these  anuna 

548 


indicated  that  paired  living  could  not  compensate  for  the  rarified  sur- 
roundings. The  cerebral  consequences  were  striking.  These  pairs  did  not 
differ  significantly  in  cortical  weight  or  enzyme  activity  from  those  who 
had  lived  in  a  solitary  state  in  these  cages,  devoid  of  interest  and  stimula- 
tion. 

As  they  created  graded  situations,  to  test  for  the  role  of  social  inter- 
action in  learning  and  brain  growth,  the  researchers  saw  that  the  extreme- 
ly impoverished  lone  animal  was  not  much  worse  off  than  pairs  in  the 
same  kind  of  cage.  Moreover,  triads,  raised  in  barren  cages,  hardly  differed 
from  their  isolated  littermates,  although  they  did  show  a  ratio  of  enzyme 
activity  which  approached  that  of  the  enriched  rats.  A  close  comparison 
of  the  visual  and  somesthetic  regions  of  the  cortex  showed  the  jesearchers 
that  the  effects  of  an  impoverished  environment  were  somewhat  mitigated 
by  placing  groups  of  lats  together.  Groups  of  10-12  rats  have  been 
studied  after  rearing  in  large  cages,  devoid  of  complexity  or  stimulation 
Sheer  numbers  do  not  appear  to  compensate  for  lack  of  environmental 
complexity.  These  rats  fall  somewhere  between  their  impoverished  and 
enriched  littermates. 

The  addition  of  toys,  variation,  and  training  appears  to  add  some 
further  element -can  sing  the  animal  to  use  his  brain  more  actively -with 
concomitant  biochemical  and  anatomical  changes  in  the  brain.  . 

Formal  training.  ~Vfh*t  is  the  role  of  pure  learning,  of  training.  In 
current  experiments,  the  laboratory  is  rearing  animals  in  isolation  in  cages 
devoid  of  toys  or  social  contact,  but  for  an  hour  or  two  each  day  the 
animal  receives  intensive  training.  Can  formal  training  alone  produce 
cerebral  growth  commensurate  with  that  observed  from  life  in  a  rich  ana 
varied  environment? 


Differential  Environment:  Impact  on  Adults 

Environment  during  the  earliest,  most  formative  years  of  life  appears  to 
leave  more  of  a  mark  than  experience  after  maturity.  Surely  this  has  ueen 
a  tenet  of  human  clinical  psychology.  It  has  been  demonstrated  in  some 
animal  experiments,  too. 

The  Berkeley  team  demonstrated  that  adult  rats  as  well  as  weanlings 
show  cerebral  effects  of  experience.  Therefore,  the  cerebral  consequences 
of  environment  were  not  merely  effects  of  normal  growth  processes 
accelerated  by  the  impact  of  environment  during  infancy. 

Between  the  time  of  weaning  (at  about  25  days)  and  105  days, ,a i  rafs 
brain  grows  appreciably.  If  he  is  kept  in  a  standard  colony  cage    here  will 
be  a  20-percent  increase  in  cortical  weight  and  a  4<H»rGent  mere, 
rest  of  the  brain  by  the  time  the  animal  is  105  days  old.  Alter 
growth  is  observed.  In  the  next  80  days  there  will  be _  n 
increment  in  brain  weight.  Since  brain  growth  contnbnt 
days,  rats   of  this  age  were  selected  for  exposure  to 
enriched  environments.  They  had  been  sexually  matim 


Adult  rats,  left  foi  80  days  in  an  enriched  or  impoverished  envi 
ment,  diverge  even  more  than  young  rats  in  their  ultimate  cortical  weif 
The  effects  of  enriched  enviionment  upon  adult  rats  do,  however,  di 
slightly  from  the  effects  on  young  nits.  Adults  gain  more  toliil  b 
weight  than  does  the  immature  animal,  and  they  show  some  weight  g;ii 
the  subcortical  legions. 

This  difference  between  young  and  old  is  notable  in  assays  of  on/} 
activity.  The  older  animals  show  a  more  pronounced  drop  in  their  eorl 
AChE  activity.  Nonetheless,  the  adult  rat  exhibits  an  cncouiaf 
plasticity,  suggesting  that  the  effects  of  mental  activity  and  a  ehallenj 
environment  can  be  induced  long  after  maturity.  The  cerebral  $m\ 
produced  environmentally  in  this  laboratory  would  not  seem  to  be  ;i  ro- 
of accelerated  maturation,  since  enriched  adult  rats,  compared  willi  IF 
adult  isolates,  showed  brain  growth  as  did  the  young. 

Nor  are  these  effects  of  differential  environment  restricted  U>  rt 
Comparable  studies  with  mice  have  offeied  very  similar  results. 


Heredity  and  Environmental  Manipulation 

Within  any  family,  variations  among  children  vividly  display  how  JMHI 
an  individual's  response  to  his  surroundings  depends  upon  liis  inherit 
structure.  Inbred  rats  show  only  slight  variation  among  litlerinutos.  Ho 
ever,  inbred  strains  differ  exceedingly  from  each  other. 

Genetic  studies  and  selective  breeding  have  played  an  importnnl  pint 
the  laboratory's  program  during  the  last  dozen  years.  Using  perfoiman 
on  certain  learning  tests  as  a  criterion  for  brightness  or  dulliujs-i.  (1 
laboratory  developed  two  stiains;  One  that  was  consistently  hriphl,  II 
other  consistently  dull.  In  each  strain,  an  impoverished  and  nn 
littermate  will  show  the  behavioral,  biochemical,  and  anatomical  i 
of  environment.  However,  some  rat  strains  show  a  greater  bra  In 
and  enzymatic  change  than  others  when  exposed  to  environment 
complexity  and  training. 

When  240  rats-24  from  each  of  10  different  strains-worts  tested  o 
reversal  discrimination,  their  errors  were  compared  with  bruin  iinalysc 
The  individual  rats  who  made  the  most  errors  had  a  smaller  cortex  rcliitu 
to  the  rest  of  their  brains.  Their  ratio  of  AChE/ChE  activity  was  ah 
closer  to  that  of  the  rat  from  an  impoverished  cage.  Even  within  ;my  give 
strain,  there  was  a  consistent  correlation  between  an  animal's  pcrlbmuik 
and  the  relative  weight  of  his  cortex  and  his  enzyme  ratio.  The  brigfi 
animals  more  nearly  resembled  animals  from  an  enriched  environment. " 

Both  heredity  and  environment  appear  to  influence  an  animal'! 
adaptiveness  and  problem -solving  ability  through  common  bioclicmka1 
and  anatomical  pathways  in  the  brain.  Whether  from  onvkomiK'nU 
manipulation  or  endowment,  the  good  problem  solver  has  a  brain  with  J 
cortex  that  is  larger  in  proportion  to  the  rest  of  his  brain  and  ii  ratio  of 
enzyme  activity  that  now  seems  to  characterize  the  efficient  learner 

550 


Other   experiments    have    confirmed    the  correspondence  between  the 
laboratory's  brain  measures  and  problem-solving  capacities. 

In  1962  the  team  left  littermates  in  their  extreme  environments  for 
only  30  days.  Then  they  regrouped  them  in  colony  cages,  under  the  care 
of  an  experimenter  who  had  no  way  of  knowing  which  rats  had  been 
enriched  and  which  impoverished.  For  10  days  they  received  preparatory 
training  on  test  apparatus  and  were  acclimated  to  doing  without  food  so 
that  they  could  be  given  a  food  reward  on  their  performance  tests.  In 
order  to  maintain  control  for  possible  weight  loss  due  to  food  deprivation, 
a  third  group  of  littermates  was  maintained  with  food  and  water  always 
available.  These  rats  received  no  training  or  testing  and,  as  it  turned  out, 
body  weight  was  not  a  factoi. 

The  enriched  and  impoverished  animals  were  then  tested  m  the 
hypothesis  apparatus.  On  their  first  trials  they  appeared  similar.  But  these 
were  easy  runs,  merely  asking  that  they  run  down  the  lighted  side  at  each 
choice  point.  When  the  cues  were  reversed  and  the  problem  became  more 
difficult,  the  impoverished  group  performed  poorly,  the  animals  many 
times  taking  the  wrong  turn.  The  enriched  group  performed  significantly 
better, 

Only  30  days  of  environmental  difference  had  made  a  pronounced 
difference  This  was  especially  striking,  since  the  impoverished  group  had 
received  the  benefits  of  10  days'  pretraining  and  handling.  This  short 
period  of  relative  stimulation  probably  attenuated  the  differences  between 
the  two  groups. 

After  the  discrimination-reversal  test  had  been  scored  for  errors,  brain 
analyses  showed  that  there  was  a  correlation  between  the  ^mber  of 
errors  an  animal  made  and  the  size  of  his  cortex  relative  to  the  test  of  h.s 
br™  Ratios  of  enzyme  activity  also  distinguished  the  poor  performer 
from  his  more  efficient  littermates.  On  the  brain  measures  _the  re  was  no 
loneer  any  difference  between  the  two  groups,  md  eating  that  the 
Imp^eSd  group  had  benefited  from  their  10  days  of  pretrammg  and 
the  20  days  of  testing. 

Environment  and  Other  Brain  Components 


!„  the  course  of  a  long  and  varied 
been  so  consistent  as  to  become  predicta*.     n  n 

solving  scores,  the  experimenters  can   s  ™f  ~™  ^e  rest  of  the 
enzmes  in  the  animal's  brain  and  the  ratio  of  ™™*       a     rebred 


enzymes 


This  has  bean  borne  out  ^  gen^c^u^  -^  jmal  exposed  to 

capacity  is  enhanced. 


551 


The  laboratory  has  traced  other  biochemical  systems  as  u 
Hexokinase,  an  enzyme  important  in  cell  metabolism,  did  not  seem  tc 
altered  by  differential  experience.  Nor  did  serotonin  activity  appear 
change.  In  other  laboratories,  scientists  have  used  differential  envir 
ments  to  measure  for  changes  in  other  brain  constituents. 

Dr.  Edward  Geller  and  a  team  of  scientists  at  UCLA  have  found  tl 
the  transmitter  substance,  norepinephrine  (also  known  as  noradrenali 
exists  in  greater  concentration  in  the  brains  of  enriched  rats.  An  assay 
specific  brain  regions  has  revealed,  however,  that  the  impoverished  anim; 
had  about  five  times  as  much  norepinephrine  in  a  subcortical  regi< 
sometimes  described  as  the  brain's  chemical  storehouse-the  cauda 

nucleus. 

The  Berkley  team  had  initially  expected   to  find  that  protein  met 
bolism  increased  in  the  brain  as  a  result  of  mental  activity  and  tin 
expected  to  find  higher  protein  turnovei  in  the  brains  of  enriched  rat 
Percent  protein  did  not  differentiate  the  enriched  from  the  impoverishe. 
groups.  Recently,  however,  Dr.  Joseph  Altman  and  his  MIT  coworkei 
have  found  that  protein  turnover  is  actually  lower  in  the  enriched  animals 
They  injected  radioactive  leucine  into  their  enriched  and  impoverishet 
littermates.  Leucine  is  an  amino  acid  used  throughout  the  brain  in  flu 
construction  of  protein.  Cells  that  utilized  the  radioactive  leucine  created 
dark  spots  on  a  photographic  emulsion,  On  examination,  the  slides  were 
a  surprise,  for  the  brains  of  the  impoverished  animals  had  absoibed  the 
most  of  the  radioactive  label  and  thus  indicated  a  higher  rate  of  protein 
metabolism.  Altman  and  his  co workers  have  speculated  that  this  high  rate 
of  protein  metabolism  may  be  a  sign  of  stress  and  that  the  isolated  animals 
may  have  experienced  stress  each  time  they  were  handled  in  the  labora- 
tory. Perhaps  a  lower  rate  of  brain  metabolism  is  a  sign  of  greater  ef- 
ficiency. 

Memory  is,  inevitably,  a  crucial  factor  in  learning*  and  some  ex- 
perimenters have  enhanced  retention  and  speed  of  learning  in  animals  by 
chemicals.  Under  certain  conditions  oral  doses  of  magnesium  pemolinc 
have  been  reported  to  cause  rats  to  learn  conditioned  responses  four  to 
five  times  faster  than  their  untreated  controls.  Moreover,  the  treated  rats 
showed  no  signs  of  forgetting  after  2  weeks.  Recently  this  drug  has  been 
tested  on  senile  patients,  and  preliminary  reports  suggest  that  it  may 
improve  memory. 


Conclusion 

From  many  quarters,  diverse  scientists  of  behavior  are  beginning  to 
illustrate  the  same  proposition.  An  individual's  behavior  depends  upon  the 
anatomical  and  chemical  attributes  of  his  brain.  To  a  large  extent  this  is 
dictated  by  heredity,  but  many  of  the  brain's  characteristics  can  be  altered 
by  experience  as  well  as  by  drugs. 

A  rich  and  complex  environment  would  seem  to  cause  brain  growth,  In 
infancy  and  also  in  adulthood.  Some  initial  evidence  suggests  that  it  is 

552 


possible    to    selectively    modify    the   brain    by   selective   environmental 
demands.  Blinded  animals,  for  instance,  have  shown  some  development  in 
the  visual  cortex  after  an  interval  in  an  enriched  surrounding.  Since  adult 
animals  also  show  brain  plasticity,  it  would  seem  that  people  with  brain 
damage   might  be   rehabilitated   by  special   training  designed   to  cause 
growth  in  specific  portions  of  the  brain.  A  recent  study  of  elderly  patients 
with  aphasia  indicates  that  brain-damaged  persons  can  be  i cached  and 
possibly  rehabilitated  by  carefully  designed  training.  Aphasics,  who  are 
unable  to  read  or  understand  speech,  have  been  given  intensive  training  in 
visual    discrimination,    a    fundamental   function    in    leading.   Following 
automated  training,  they  have  shown  improved  performance  in  discrimina- 
ting the  geometric  shapes  that  make  up  the  English  alphabet  and  have 
given  signs  of  retention  on  subsequent  followup  tests.  It  appears  that 
programmed  training  might  become  an  effective  tool,  helping  disabled 
patients    to  function  once  more,  perhaps  by  causing  growth  in  intact 
portions  of  the  brain.  Surely,  the  Berkeley  studies  suggest  that  experience 
can  improve  a  creature's  capacity  for  learning  and  adapting,  by  piomoting 
change   in  certain  anatomical  and  biochemical  pathways  of  the  brain. 
These   appear  to  be  pathways  by  which  cultivation  can  enlarge  the  in- 
dividual's capacities,  whatever  his  hereditary  limits  initially  may  seem  to 
be, 

These  studies  have  interesting  implications  for  education.  Perhaps  we 
can  begin  to  understand  factors  in  the  development  of  the  young,  which 
at  present  yield  puzzling  statistics.  Many  studies  of  birth  order  and  life 
achievement  (for  instance)  suggest  that  the  first-born  child  in  a  family  is 
more  likely  to  be  gifted  and  eminent.  Twins,  indeed,  are  found  to  have 
lower  IQ's  than  single-born  children  or  pairs  of  children  spaced  farther 
apart.  On  the  other  hand,  studies  conducted  in  orphanages  suggest  that 
institutionally    reared    babies    lag   behind    their    counterparts   raised   in 
families.    Could    these   differences  reflect   the   relative  enrichment  and 
stimulation  the  children  receive  as  they  are  growing?  Does  the  first  child 
in  a  family  receive  far  more  attention  and  handling  on  the  average  than 
the  subsequent  children?  A  scattering  of  studies  suggests  that  this  may  be 
the    case.   One  cannot  probe  the  development  of  the  cortex  in  human 
Infants;   still    a  difference  has  been  seen  in  the  visual  performance  of 
orphan  infants  who  were  given  visual  stimulation  and  toys  in  their  cribs. 
Cross-cultural  studies  have  indicated  that  the  early  precocity  of  a  very 
young   child  in  Uganda,  for  instance,  may  be  related  to  the  constant 
company  and  attention  of  the  mother. 

A  dramatic  case  in  point  has  been  reported  recently.  Thirty  years  ago 
Dr.  Harold  M.  Skeels  of  NIMH  experimented  with  13  toddlers  who  were 
classed  as  mentally  retarded.  They  were  orphans  in  an  Iowa  institution, 
where  the  usual  procedure  was  to  keep  retarded  children  for  a  while  and 
then  transfer  them  to  a  special  institution.  By  chance  two  youngsters  got 
transferred  early.  Within  a  year  they  were  mentally  normal.  Because  they 
were  much  younger  they  attracted  a  great  deal  of  attention  and  care  from 
the  older  inmates,  and  this  apparently  accounted  for  their  improved  intel- 
ligence. Dr.  Skeels  noted  that  the  orphanage  was  so  efficient  that  infants 

553 


received  little  individual  attention.  He  placed  13  retarded  orphans  as 
"houseguests"  in  a  state  institution  at  a  very  early  age.  Three  of  them 
were  categorized  as  imbeciles.  Eleven  of  the  children  so  profited  from  the 
extra  play  and  care  that  they  became  "normally"  intelligent,  were  put  up 
for  adoption,  and  later  became  self-supporting  middle-class  adults.  A 
control  group  of  the  same  age  had  been  left  in  the  orphanage.  These 
children  had  average  intelligence.  After  a  number  of  years  both  groups 
were  tested.  The  normal  children,  within  the  orphanage,  lost  IQ  points, 
whereas  the  supposedly  retarded  children  had  gained  in  IQ,  The  two 
groups  had,  indeed,  switched  positions,  and  one  child  who  began  with  a 
rating  of  good  average  intelligence  had  become  an  imbecile  by  age  1 9, 
after  a  life  in  the  orphanage.  The  impact  of  environment  on  intelligence 
was  heartbreakingly  palpable. 

In  the  rearing  and  education  of  our  young,  we  can  now  see  that  the 
environment  we  provide  may  enhance  or  retard  brain  growth— and  perhaps 
intelligence.  However,  one  cannot  liken  the  culturally  deprived  person  to 
the  impoverished  laboratory  rat.  Except  for  those  people  who  are  deaf 
and  blind,  or  seriously  handicapped,  a  vacuum  of  experience  is  rare. 
Rather,  the  experience  of  our  young  is  random.  We  have  done  only  little 
to  discover  how  a  human  education  should  be  programed  and  placed  to 
maximize  the  potential  of  each  individual.  It  is  no  news  to  say  that  the 
resources  of  our  population  could  be  increased  by  an  education  that 
induced  mental  growth.  Now  this  same  proposition  has  been  put  into 
physiological  language.  We  can  deliberately  cultivate  more  effective  brains 
and  cause  growth  in  the  cerebral  cortex. 

Research  Grants.  MH  1292,  MH  7903 

Dates  of  Interviews:  December  1965,  May  1966 

References: 

Altmaii,  J.  Are  new  neurons  formed  in  the  brains  of  adult  mammals?  Science,  1962, 

135,3509. 
Altman.  J.  Autoradiographic  investigation  of  cell  proliferation  in  the  brains  of  rats  and 

c9.ts.Anat.  Rec.t  1963,  145,4. 
Altman,  J.  Differences  in  the  utilization  on  tritiated  leucine  by  single  neurones  in 

normal  and  exercised  rats:  on  autoradiographic  investigation  with  microdensi- 

tometry.  Nature,  1963,  199,4895, 

Altman,  J.  The  use  of  fine-resolution  autoradiography  in  neurological  and  psycho- 
biological  research.  In  Haley  &  Snider  (Eds.),  Response  of  the  nervous  system  to 

ionizing  radiation.  Little,  Brown,  1964. 
Altman,  J.  Autoradiographic  examination  of  behaviorally  induced  changes  in  the 

protein  and  nucleic  acid  metabolism  in  the  brain.  In  F.  A.  O.  Schmitt  (Ed.), 

Macro  molecules  and  behavior. 
Altman,  J.,  and  Das,  G.  D.  Autoradiographic  examination  of  the  effects  of  enriched 

environment  on  the  rate  of  glial  multiplication  in  the  adult  rat  brain.  Nature,  1 964, 

204,4964. 
Altman,  J.,  and  Das,  G,  D.  Postnatal  origin  of  microneui'ones  in  the  rat  brain.  Nature, 

1965,207,5000. 
Altman,  J.,  and  Das,  G.  D,  Autoradiographic  and  histological  evidence  of  postnatal 

hippocampal  neurogenesis  in  rats.  J.  Camp.  Neural,  1965,  124,  3. 
Altman ,  J.,  Das.,  G.  D.,  and  Chang,  J.  Behavioral  manipulations  and. protein  metabo- 
lism of  the  brain:  effects  of  visual  training  on  the  utilization  of  Leucine-H3. 

Physiology  and  Behavior,  1966,  vol.  l.Pergamon  Press. 

554 


Bennett,  E.  L.,  Crossland,  J.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Rosenzweig,  M  R.  Strain  differences  in 
acetylcholine  concentrations  in  rat  brain.  Nature,  1960,  187,787. 

Bennett,  E.  L.t  Diamond,  M.  C.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  Chemical  and 
anatomical  plasticity  of  brain  Science,  1964,  146,610-619. 

Bennett,  E.  L.,  Diamond,  M.  C  ,  Krech,  D.,  and  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  Heredity  environ- 
ment, learning,  and  the  brain.  AAAS  Symposium,  Behavior,  Brain,  and  Bio- 
chemistry. Berkeley,  Calif.,  Dec.  28,  1965. 

Bennett,  B.  L.,  Drori,  J.  B.,  Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Abraham,  S.  Hex- 

okinase  activity  in  brain./.  Biol  Chem.,  1962,237,  1758-1763. 
Bennett,  E.   L.,    Drori,  J.  B.,  Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M,  R.,  Diamond,  M.  C.,  and 
Abraham,  S.   Determination  of  hexokmase  activity  in  brain.  Fed.  Proc.,  1961,  20, 
343  (abstract). 

Bennett,  E.  L.,  Herbert,  M.,  Rosenzweig,  M  R.,  and  Krech,  D.  Genetic  selection  of  rats 
for  cerebral  acetylcholme  concentration.  Proc.  Sixth  Int.  Congr.  Biochem ,  New 
York,  1964. 

Bennett,  E.  L.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  Reliability  and  regional  specificity 
of  cerebral  effects  of  environmental  complexity  and  training.  J.  Comp,  Physiol 
Psychol,  1964,57,440-441. 

Bennett,  E.  L.,  Rosenzweig,  M,  R.,  Krech,  D,,  Karlsson,  H.,  Dye,  N.,  and  Ohlander,  A. 
Individual,  strain,  and  age  differences  in  cholinesterase  activity  of  the  rat  brain.  /. 
ffeurochem. ,  1958,3,144-152. 

Das,  G.  D.f  and  Altman,  J.  Behavioral  manipulations  and  protein  metabolism  of  the 
brain:  effects  of  motor  exercise  on  the  utilization  of  Leucine-H3.  Physiology  and 
Betiavtoi,  1966,  vol.  1,  Permagon  Press. 

Das,  G.  D.,  and  Altman,  J,  Behavioral  manipulations  and  protein  metabolism  of  the 
brain:    effects    of    restricted    and   enriched  environments  on  the  utilization  of 
Leucjne-H3,  Physiology  and  Behavior.  1966,  vol.  1 ,  Permagon  Press. 
Diamond,  M.  C.,  Diamond,  R.  M.,  Bennett,  E.  L.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Rosenzweig,  M.  R. 
Distribution    of  acetylcholinesterase  in  cerebral  cortical  tissue  slices  from  maze 
bright  and  maze  dull  sliains  of  rats.  Anat,  Rec.,  1961,  139,221  (abstract). 
Diamond ,  M,  C.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Krech,  D.  Relationships  between  body  weight 
and  skull  development  in  rats  raised  in  enriched  and  impoverished  conditions.  J. 
Exp,  Zool.,  1965,  160,29-35. 
Krech,  D.  Heredity,  environment,  brain  and  problem  solving,  Internatl.  Cong.  Psych., 

Moscow,  August  1966. 

Krech,   D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Bennett,  E.  L.,  and  Krueckel,  B.  Enzyme  concentra- 
tions in  the  brain  and  adjustive  behavior  patterns.  Science,  1954,  120,  994-996. 
Krech,   D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Dimensions  of  discrimination  and 
level  of  chalinest erase  activity  in  the  cerebral  cortex  of  the  rat  J.  Comp.  Physiol. 
Psychol,  1956,49,261-268. 
Krech,    D.,   Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Cholinesterase  activity.  Science, 

1958,  12S,  1  176. 

Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L  Correlations  between  brain 
cholinesterase  and  brain  weight  within  two  strains  of  rats.Amer.J.  Physio!.,  1959, 
196,  31-32.  ,  ,  ., 

Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Effects  of  environmental  complexity 

and  training  on  brain  chemistry.  J.  Comp.  Physiol.  Psycho!,,  I960,  53,  509-519. 
Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Inter  hemispheric  effects  of  cortical 

lesions  on  brain  biochemistry.  Science,  1960,  132,  352-353.  .     .      •  , 

Krech,  D.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Relations  between  brain  chemistry 
and  problem  solving,  among  rats  raised  in  enriched  and  impoverished  environments. 
J.  Comp.  Physiol  Psycho!.,  1962,55,801-807.  . 

Krech,  D."  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  and  Bennett,  E,  L.  Effects  of  complex  environment  and 
blindness  on  rat  brain.  Arch.  Neurol,  1963,8,403-412.  hphflvinr  APA 

Rosenzweig,  M.  R.  Environmental  complexity,  cerebral  change,  and  behavior,  ArA 
Chicago,  Sept.  5,  1965. 

555 


Rosenzweig,  M.  R   Evidence  for  anatomical  and  chemical  changes  m  the  brain  dm 

primary  learning.  Symposium  on  Biological  Bases  of  Memory  Traces,  Intern 

Cong.  Psych.,  Moscow,  August  1966 
Rosenzweig,  M    R.,  Bennett.  E.  L.,  and  Krech,  D.  Cerebral  effects  of  envjronmer 

complexity  and  training  among  adult  rats  /   Comp.  Physiol  Psychol,  J964, 

438-439. 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.   L.  Acetylcholine  metabolism  a 

behavior  ot  rats.  Science,  1959,  129,  62-64. 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  A  search  for  relations  between  brs 

chemistry  and  behavior  Psychol  Bull,  1960,  57,476-492. 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  Bennett,  E.  L.,  and  Diamond,  M.  C.  Effects  of  enviro 

mental  complexity  and  training  on  brain  chemistry  and  anatomy:  a  replication  ai 

extension,/.  Comp.  Physiol.  Psychol.,  1962,  55,  429-437. 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Effects  of  environmental  cotnplcxil 

and  training  on  brain  anatomy  and  cholmeslerase  m  rat.  Fed.  Pwc,,  1962,  21,3j 

(abstract) 
Rosenzweig,   M.    R.,    Krech,    D.,   and    Bennett,    E.    L,,    Effects  of  environment! 

complexity  and  training  on  brain  chemistry  and  anatomy  among  mature  rats.  Fee 

Proc.,  1963,  22,  515  (abstract). 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Effects  of  differential  experience  o, 

brain.  AChE  and  ChE  and  brain  anatomy  in  the  rat,  as  a  function  of  strain  and  age 

A mer.  Psychologist,  1963,  18,430. 
Rosenzweig,  M.   R.,   Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Strain  differences  in  cerebra 

responses  to  environmental  complexity  and  training.  Fed.  Proc.,  1964,  23,  25; 

(abstract). 
Rosenzweig,  M.  R,,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Modifying  brain  chemistry  and 

anatomy  by  enrichment  or  impoverishment  of  experience.  In  G.  Newton  and  S. 

Levine  (Eds.),  Readings  in  early  experience,  1 965,  in  piess. 
Woolley,  D.  E.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  Bennett,  E.  L.,  and  Timiras,  P.  S.  Strain 

and  sex  differences  in  threshold  and  pattern  of  electroshock  convulsions  in  rals. 

The  Physiologist,  1 960,  3,  1  82  (abstract). 
Woolley,  D.  B.,  Timiras,  P.  S.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D,,  and  Bennett,  E.  L  Strain 

and  sex  differences  m  electroshock  convulsions  of  the  lat.  Nature,  J961,  190, 

515-516. 
Woolley,  D.  E.,  Timiras,  P.  S.,  Rosenzweig,  M.  R.,  Krech,  D.,  and  Bennett,  E.  L.  Strain 

differences  in  seizme  responses  and  brain  cholinesterase  activity  in  rats.  Proc,  we. 

exp,  biol  Med.,  1963,  112,  781-785. 


556 


Investigator- 
James  L.  McGaugh,  Ph.  D. 

University  of  California 
Irvine,  Calif. 

Prepared  by: 

Gay  Luce 


Introduction 


The  suivival  of  mankind  may  now  hang  upon  how  well  we  can  educate 
each  coming  generation,  since  the  capacity  for  sophisticated  and  coopera- 
tive behavior  has  become  the  essential  of  modern  civilization.  Ironically, 
GUI  educational  institutions  lag  behind  us.  Agriculture  and  medicine  have 
been  transformed  by  modern  scientific  research  while  the  schools  remain 
virtually   untouched  by  the  20th  century.  One  aspect  of  the  scientific 
Harvest    that   might  indeed  influence  the  training  of  the  young  is  the 
GP  pl°Jatlon  of  biological  bases  for  learning  and  memory.  We  can  no  longer 
atlord  the  luxury  of  ignoring  important  problems  of  memory  and  their 
implications  for  the  manner  in  which  we  teach. 

The  social  implications  of  some  researches  in  this  area  are  just  beginning 
to    be   known.   Experiments  with  rats  have  divulged  a  genetic  base  for 
learning.  As  University  of  California  Professors  Krech  and  Rosenzweig  and 
their  associates  have  demonstrated,  it  is  possible  to  influence  the  learning 
ability  of  rodents  by  enriching  or  impoverishing  the  environment,  thereby 
also  altering  brain  biochemistry.  Without  stimulation,  there  can  be  little 
learning.  Without  memory,  there  can  be  no  learning.  There  appear  to  be 
many  processes  in  memory,  some  related  to  short-term  memory,  some 
related    to   long-term  storage,  and  others  related  to  retrieval.  There  are 
sorne  patients  with  brain  damage,  for  example,  who  appear  to  be  normal, 
with  unaffected  IQ's,  yet  they  cannot  learn  anything  new,  not  even  a  new 
iiome  address.  Dr.  McGaugh  and  his  associates  have  used  electroconvulsive 
sliock  to  produce  a  similar  amnesia  for  new  experience  in  animals.  By  this 
device  he  has  seen  evidence  that  there  may  be  two  separate  stages  in  the 
Implantation  of  any  memory. 

The  potentiality  for  improving  education  by  using  such  research  has 
been  demonstrated  by  Prof.  Millard  Madsen  of  UCLA.  He  has  shown  how 
knowledgeable  timing  of  information  will  permit  children  with  low  IQ 
scores  to  learn  quite  as  well  and  almost  as  fast  as  children  with  high  IQ's. 

Tn  studying  biochemical  influences  upon  memory,  Doctor  McGaugh  has 
recently  found  that  biological  time  of  day  may  also  influence  learning 
toeliavior. 

557 


History 

In  1917,  the  psychologist  William  Lashley  observed  that  rats  would 
learn  to  run  a  maze  with  more  celerity  than  usual  if  they  were  fiist  given 
low  doses  of  strychnine  sulfate.  The  implication  that  drugs  might  in- 
fluence learning  or  be  useful  in  exploring  memory  processes  was  ignored 
for  several  decades.  Then,  stimulant  drugs,  known  as  analeptics,  were 
again  observed  to  facilitate  learning. 

In  recent  experiments  strychnine,  picrotoxin,  pentylenetetrazole,  and 
diazadamantanol  have  been  used  on  animals  performing  prescribed  and 
measurable  tasks.  Some  have  been  discrimination  problems,  in  which  the 
animals  learned  to  choose  among  alternative  paths  in  a  maze  in  order  to 
reach  a  goal  and  to  discriminate  between  black  and  white  gates,  while  in 
otheis  they  had  to  escape  from  a  situation  in  which  they  would  be 
punished  or  learn  restraint  in  order  to  avoid  electric  shock.  The  situations, 
while  limited,  were  precisely  controlled  and  therefore  quantifiable.  In 
most  of  the  early  studies,  animals  were  first  given  stimulants  a  few 
minutes  before  their  first  encounter  with  the  training,  and  were  trained 
under  drug  influence.  Did  the  drug  influence  the  process  we  call  memory? 
Perhaps  not.  Perhaps  it  improved  performance  by  making  the  animal  more 
attentive,  by  sharpening  his  perceptions,  by  enhancing  his  motivation,  or 
by  improving  muscular  coordination.  Research  on  learning  resembles  the 
divergent  reports  from  six  blind  men  describing  an  elephant,  each  touch- 
ing a  different  part  of  the  beast.  The  impact  of  a  drug  upon  "learning"  is 
partly  dependent  upon  the  measure  of  learning.  One  easily  quantified  part 
of  performance  is  the  response  latency-how  long  it  takes  the  animal  to 
get  around  to  making  a  response,  When  this  measure  is  the  criterion  of 
learning,  a  drug  that  increases  response  speed  or  alertness  will  also  seem  to 
improve  learning.  Since  no  experimenter  can  afford  to  use  many  criteria 
of  performance,  studies  of  learning  and  memory  typically  contain  some 
knotty  problems  of  method  and  measurement.  Doctor  McGaugh  and  his 
associates  began  to  train  unclrugged  animals,  but  gave  them  "memory" 
tests  while  they  were  drugged.  Perhaps  drugs  altered  dimensions  of  per- 
formance, although  they  did  not  act  upon  memory  processes  during  learn- 
ing. It  soon  appeared  that  the  memory  process,  itself,  was  exceedingly 
subtle. 


Retrograde  Amnesia 

One  means  of  exploring  memory  is  by  training  a  creature  on  a  narrowly 
prescribed  performance  and  administering  drugs  or  convulsive  shock  at 
intervals  directly  after  the  training.  Will  they  interfere  with  the  consolida- 
tion of  memory?  Does  it  matter  how  soon  after  training  the  shock  or  drug 
is  given?  In  the  course  of  many  such  experiments,  the  grantee  and  his 
associates  found  that  animals  shocked  immediately,  or  at  short  intervals 
after  training,  seemed  later  not  to  recall.  By  strategically  varying  their 
treatment  of  the  animals,  they  saw  that  training  or  experience  seemed  to 

558 


initiate  a  kind  of  potential  residue  for  memory.  Yet  this  potentially  per- 
manent  memory  would  remain  labile  for  long  periods,  perhaps  hours, 
before  being  permanently  etched  into  the  neural  code  of  memory.  These 
studies  in  which  memory  was  impaired  by  drug  or  shock  interference  led 
Dr.  McGaugh  to  wonder  about  enhancing  memory  in  a  similar  manner.  If 
there  is  a  long  period  after  training  in  which  memory  processes  are  active, 
a  memory-enhancing  drug  could  be  given  in  this  interval  after  training  and 
should  improve  performance  or  make  the  animal  resistant  to  calculated 
amnesia-electroshock. 


Drugs  After  Training 

The    training  procedures   were  straightforward.  Animals  were  condi- 
tioned to  push  a  lever  or  avoid  a  grid,  etc.  Then,  after  training,  they  were 
injected  ^  with  drugs.  After  a  suitable  interval  allowing  the  drugs  to  be 
metabolized,  the  animals  were  tested  on  the  original  procedure.  A  number 
of  drugs  appeared  to  enhance  performance  when  injected  after  training- 
and  by  implication  seemed  to  be  acting  upon  the  mysterious  processes  of 
memory.    Strychnine,   picrotoxin  (at  low  doses),  and  amphetamine  all 
seemed  to  enhance  the  learning  of  animals  who  received  doses  directly 
after  training;   on  tests  they  outperformed  the  animals  who  had  received 
only  a  placebo. 

^  During  the  last  few  years,  several  drugs  have  been  given  to  animals  on  a 
wide   range  of  learning  tasks,  measured  by  various  criteria.  Clearly,  the 
outcome  is  some  blend  of  the  kind  of  learning  (it  is  vastly  different  to 
learn  to  discriminate  between  two  colors  than  to  learn  to  avoid  a  shock  at 
the  toll  of  a  bell),  the  experimental  conditions,  the  drug,  and  the  amount 
of  the  drug  used.  Results  from  laboratories  around  the  country  are  not  all 
in  accordance,  but  most  of  the  evidence  suggests  that  certain  drugs  en- 
hance learning.  If  so,  presumably,  they  are  acting  upon  memoiy  storage  in 
Its    labile  period.  Presumably,  these  same  drugs  would  do  nothing  for 
memory  if  they  were  injected  at  some  maximum  time  after  training.  By 
carefully   graded  experiments,  therefore,  one  might  expect  to  delineate 
How  long  the  labile  period  of  memory  lasts. 

The  procedure  in  this  laboratory  was  straightforward.  One  group  of 
animals  would  be  injected  with  a  drug  5  seconds  after  they  responded  to  a 
learning  procedure.  Another  group  would  be  injected  after  1  minute,  still 
another  after  5  minutes,  and  so  on.  During  1962,  Doctor  McGaugh  was 
injecting  strychnine  at  various  intervals  after  training:  he  found  that  one 
strain  of  mice  "learned"  better  if  injected  immediately  after  training,  yet 
injections  given  a  half  hour  after  training  were  ineffective.  On  a  discrimi- 
nation task,  one  group  of  mice  showed  the  greatest  facilitation-by  com- 
parison with  undrugged  controls-if  they  were  injected  between  5  and  1<: 
minutes  before  training. 

The  results  of  these  studies  of  effects  of  time  o 
drugs  indicate  that  the  effects  are  time-dependei 
of  the  facilitating  effect  decreases  with  the  intervi 


lllt-Sv    I  >Y\_)    Hll^**   \J  L    ^-rin^'i'^'v    ^F-^*-v»**      •  -  •  j     »,  *„  -^  --u    j-  i     .-  -  -     - 

that  memory  storage  processes  are  susceptible  to  both 

and  impairing  influences  for  a  relatively  long  period  of  time  folio 

ing  training. 


Environment 

The  elusiveness  and  delicacy  of  the  process  of  memory  and  (lie  sen- 
tivity  of  rodents-has  forced  the  investigators  to  take  nil  kinds  ofprcviu 
tions  against  slamming  doors  and  disturbance  in  the  laboratory.  Tin."  ML-L- 
for  constant  temperature,  quiet,  etc.,  was  underscored  by  an  cxpuiimcn 
in  which  mice,  given  strychnine,  weie  given  a  discrimination  problem  ;IIK 
were  disrupted  by  environmental  stimulation.  Like  .sUuluiMs  Irymji  u 
memorize  a  lesson  in  an  "acid-rock"  discotheque  under  strobe  liphK  tail] 
these  mice  were  rocked  back  and  forth  in  their  cuge,  exposed  to  flash JIIL 
lights  and  bursts  of  sound  for  20  minutes.  On  tests,  il  WHS  Hie  oilier  lull 
of  the  animais-who  remained  in  dark  quiet  cagos-  -who  showed  unhnncfd 
learning  from  strychnine  while  the  stimulated  animals  did  worse  Hun 
controls.  It  was  an  encouragement  for  control  in  the  learning 
and  perhaps  also  a  hint  for  humans. 


Drug  Attenuation  of  Retrograde  Amnesia 

Inevitably,  the  investigator  wondered  whether  strychnine  itml  oilier 
drugs  enhanced  learning  by  accelerating  the  rate  a|  which  Jiiemory  f,Viu'* 
were  consolidated  into  permanent  memory.  If  so,  the  (IniKs  sliouhl  /in- 
vent or  attenuate  the  kind  of  retrograde  amnesia  (Jim  is  causwl  l>y  utiivtri. 
sive  shock.  Animals  were  given  saline  solution  or  strychnine  just  he  I  ore  ur 
just  after  training:  then,  within  a  few  minutes  each  uniimil  WHS  shockviJ  A 
day  iater  each  mouse  was  tested  on  the  training  (ask.  The  i1miMii 
•niniate  die!  a  little  better  on  tests  than  did  the  con.rols  who  r««.  v  S 


Recently  the  experimenters  have  used  a  diffurent  kind 
^ar^tTS^  SUmdCm  '^  "  teSl  Of  IM^«- 


Plairo"» 


The  mouse,  seeking  the  comfort  and  security  of  a  dark  box,  would  step 
in  the  hole  and  immediately  get  a  shock  on  the  foot.  Twenty-four  hours 
later  he  would  be  placed  on  the  platform  again.  Would  he  remember  that 
the  hole  leads  to  a  "forbidden"  place?  Would  he  restrain  his  natuial  urge 
for  security?  The  length  of  time  that  he  would  hesitate  on  the  platform 
before  attempting  to  enter  the  box  would  represent  a  degree  of  learning. 
This  time,  resisting  temptation,  as  it  were,  was  measured  as  the  criterion 
of  learning. 

Each  mouse  was  placed  on  the  platform,  received  a  footshock  when  it 
transgressed  the  boundary  and  then  received  electroconvulsive  shock.  For 
some,  the  shock  came  within  18  seconds;  for  some,  it  came  after  18 
seconds;  and  other  animals  received  it  an  hour  or  3  hours  later.  Some  of 
these  mice  were  on  saline  solution,  whereas  half  the  mice  received 
strychnine  either  1 0  minutes  before  or  a  minute  after  the  platform  train- 
ing. 

Twenty-four  hours  later,  each  mouse  was  put  to  the  test,  placed  on  the 
platform,  and  clocked.  The  amount  of  time  an  animal  stayed  on  the 
platform  without  moving  through  the  hole  was  directly  proportional  to 
the  amount  of  time  he  had  been  allowed  between  his  first  experience  and 
electroconvulsive  shock.  The  animals  given  shock  3  horns  after  training 
showed  to  amnesia  at  all.  Control  animals  given  shock  18  seconds  or  a 
minute  after  their  training  experience  seemed  to  have  forgotten  that  entry 
was  forbidden.  Those  pretested  with  strychnine  were  not  rendered  so 
completely  amnesic  by  shock  at  the  same  short  inteivals.  Oddly  enough, 
some  memory  also  persisted  in  the  animals  who  had  injections  of  strych- 
nine after  the  electroshock. 

If   strychnine  affects  learning  by  acclerating  consolidation  pro- 
cesses, these  injections  should  have  no  effect.  However*  as  can  be 
seen,  retention  of  these  animals  was  superior  to  that  of  controls. 
Why  should  strychnine  counteract  the  amnesia  effects  when  injected 
after  electroshock? 

In  the  next  series  of  experiments,  strychnine  was  administered  at  vary- 
ing intervals  after  the  electroshock.  Evidently  the  critical  interval  was  the 
timing  of  the  shock.  Strychnine  did  not  improve  the  retention  of  an 
animal  given  shock  8  seconds  after  training;  yet  if  shock  was  delayed  a 
minute  or  3  hours,  strychnine  made  a  difference.  In  subsequent  experi- 
ments, it  became  evident  that  the  timing  of  the  strychnine  was  also  perti- 
nent. The  longer  the  interval  between  the  drug  and  the  shock,  the  less 
effect  the  drug  had  in  improving  memory.  If  injected  9  hours  aftershock, 
it  was  totally  ineffective.  Further  studies  showed  the  same  effects  with 
other  stimulants  such  as  picrotoxin. 

It  appeared  that  these  drugs  did  not  act-as  previously  believed-by 
accelerating  memory  consolidation.  Otherwise,  why  should  they  cancel 
the  amnesic  effects  of  electroshock? 

Doctor  McGaugh  and  his  associates  conjectured  that  ther 
distinct  phases  in  memory  consolidation.  Pei 
tion  of  a  pattern  in  the  brain  which  is  then 
ma  terial  in  the  permanent  "files"  of  the  mind 


the  first  step  if  given  instantly,  within  8  seconds  of  an  experiuntY,  p 
dedicating  that  initial  pattern.  However,  if  that  first  palteni  wci 
mitted  time  to  form-taking  more  than  8  seconds—  then  shock  Mov 
second  stage  of  permanent  storage.  This  means  that  if  one  WL'IC  lo 
sufficiently  long  time  after  a  training  trial,  the  first  phase  ol'  UK 
would  be  impervious  to  erasure  by  shock.  What  shock  docs  ;it  I 
intervals,  is  to  obliterate  the  second  stage—  and  it  was  the  sec'otul  sl.i#< 
was  influenced  by  drugs. 

Drugs  might  affect  learning  in  many  ways,  and  as  we  conic  Id  it 
stand  them  it  may  be  possible  to  evolve  chemotherapies  Cor  if  ok- 
memory,  such  as  those  of  the  elderly  or  the  retarded  child,  or  tlisoi 
like  those  of  brain-damaged  patients.  In  recent  years  ninny 
have  postulated  that  the  synthesis  of  nucleic  acids  nuisl   In1 
memory.  Antibiotics  can  inhibit  the  synthesis  of  RNA,  yet 
not  impair  learning  in  animals.  Puromysin  inhibits  prole  in  synilu'iK. 
when  injected  into  the  brain  of  a  goldfish  has  produced  effects  rcscinh 
those  of  electroshock.  Some  researchers  have   postulated   Unit  im-m 
depends  upon  changing  levels  of  ncurotiansmittcrs  such  iis  acelyMjoJi 
Using  chemicals  that  effectually  raise  the  levels  of  ncutylcltoJijio  iit  i 
brain,  one  laboratory  found  that  there  were  extraordinary  cITtvls  up 
memory  -an  injection  might  eradicate  memory  of  a  task  loimuMl  I  f  Jj 
earlier  but   enhance  a  performance   learned    30   diiys   bct'oic.   rie.irl 
memory  processes  arc  time-dependent,  and  changes  occur  in  MIIIIC  rut 
terious  but  presumably  regular  order. 


A  new  dimension  has  been  added  to  the  study  of 
the  finding  that  biological  time  of  day  influences  memory- 


Daily  Rhythms  of  Memory 

In  recent  studies  with  Dr.  Gwen  Stephens,  the  grantee  has  (ouml  ih 
mice  retain  inhibitory  training  best  if  trained  at  night,  when  (J,cv  ..H 
the  peak  of  their  body  temperature  and  motor  activity  for  the  24  ton* 
indeed  .just  as  training  seems  more  vividly  impressed  at  Iliis  nmc.  ili 

?  mSft^f?    *CtS.      Sh°Ck  are  als°  more  Pronounwd  when  they  l^,,,, 
3  minutes  after  training  at  this  peak  time. 


Wilhin  l 

th       ,f  ,      int°  the  box>  on|y  to  ^  sliockcil.    -u 

*heplatform  withi»  5  seconds,  somo  seemed  not  to  mnenilv 


M  , 

and  would  step  agmn  into  the  forbidden  place.  However,  if 

ndexperience  ™  lhc  P^tfonn  after  «  del  y   el  oy 


hove  ,  r  «     e    y   e   oy 

hover  there  for  a  long  time  before  venturing  in,  A  clay  Inter  II  ev  *  ."; 

2±f  ""  l'rge  t0  eiUCr  the  d«?k  ***  com^bJc    ol  *  n 
tor  remembering  was  set  at  30  seconcls-for  If  the  anintiiJ  hwmJ 

•^  rccaiied  uic  ^*-  ""ho  r^;;; 

^^^  ™  SOT  Ioss  'n  the  strength  of  h^ 
between  30  seconds  and  zero  seconds  was  taktn  to 

562 


measure  the  loss  of  memory,  since  animals  placed  on  the  platfoim  the  fiist 
time  would  often  hesitate  for  a  few  seconds  before  exploring. 

A  recent  seiies  of  experiments  was  run  in  several  ways.  Some  animals 
never  icceived  more  than  one  experience  on  the  platform.  Others  were 
placed  there  seveial  times  in  succession.  Groups  of  animals  destined  for 
the  same  schedule  of  treatment  were  trained  and  tested  at  trough  and 
crest  of  the  24-hour  temperatuie-activity  rhythm. 

Ironically,  much  of  the  study  of  memory  and  the  impact  of  drugs  has 
been  conducted  with  rodents.  They  are  nocturnal  animals,  whose  peak 
activity  occurs  in  darkness.  Yet  these  animals  are  treated  in  most  labora- 
tories as  if  they  were  diurnal.  Usually  they  are  tested  at  times  when  they 
would  normally  be  resting  and  sleeping. 

The  influence  of  experimental  ordei  within  a  training  and  treatment 
session  was  noticed  in  60  mice  who  were  put  on  the  platform,  given 
shock,  and  later  tested.  It  seemed  to  matter  whether  the  mice  had  been  in 
the  early  part,  the  middle,  or  the  late  portion  of  an  experiment,  since  the 
training  and  treatment  of  60  animals  stretched  out  across  2  or  3  horns, 
Weie  the  animals  responding  to  noise  in  the  laboiatory,  to  lighting,  to  the 
experimenters? 

Under  the  suggestion  that  time  of  day  might  matter,  experimental  data 
for  one  study  were  submitted  to  a  double  analysis.  The  usual  analysis 
scored  groups  of  animals  for  their  retention,  grouping  them  according  to 
the  elapsed  time  between  training  and  shock.  Those  animals  shocked 
within  5  seconds  of  training  showed  almost  no  retention;  those  shocked 
after  15  seconds  or  many  minutes  later  showed  signs  of  memory  in  pro- 
portion to  the  interval  between  training  and  shock.  However,  the  neat 
correlation  between  retention  and  elapsed  time  before  shock  was  thrown 
into  complication,  if  the  data  were  grouped  differently.  If  averages  were 
lakcn  by  cage  of  four  animals  in  the  order  trained—regardless  of  the 
interval  between  training  and  shock— the  retest  scores  show  a  striking 
fluctuation.  No  longer  is  there  a  clean  straight  line,  suggesting  the  direct 
correlation  between  elapsed  time  before  shock  and  retention.  Instead,  it 
would  seem  that  physiologic  status  of  the  individual  animal  at  the  time  of 
training  and  treatment  is  a  big  factor  in  the  retention  of  the  animal- 
icgardless  of  the  length  of  time  between  training  and  shock. 

The  experimenters  discovered,  for  instance,  that  the  animals  trained  in 
the  first  20  minutes  of  a  2-hour  experiment  were  very  different  from 
those  trained  in  the  last  third  of  the  experiment.  They  showed  no  correla- 
tion between  retention  and  the  interval  between  training  and  shock.  Yet 
those  animals  processed  later  did  show  a  correlation.  Was  this  due  to  the 
experimenters,  to  noise  in  the  laboratory,  to  time  of  day? 

In  these  "time  of  day"  experiments,  the  animals  have  been  housed  in 
quiet  cages,  protected  from  disturbance,  and  maintained  at  a  constant 
temperature  on  a  rigid  lighting  schedule.  The  mice  live  in  darkness  from  4 
p.m.  to  4  a.m.  and  in  light  between  4  a.m.  and  4  p.m.  This  entrains  the 
animals'  temperature  rhythm  so  that  at  the  1  p.m.  experimental  hour  they 
are  at  trough  values;  at  the  9  p.m.  session  they  are  at  crest. 

563 


Time  of  Day  and  Amnesia 

Recently,  an  experimental  population  of  72  mice  was  divided  into  six 
groups.  Half  were  processed  at  1  p.m.,  the  middle  of  the  animals'  res! 
period;  the  other  half  were  processed  at  9  p.m.  at  about  the  peak  of 
activity,  In  each  time  period,  for  each  of  three  conditions  there  was  a 
control  group  of  animals  who  received  only  a  sham  electroshock,  while 
the  others  were  put  into  convulsion.  Each  animal  went  through  three  trials 
on  the  platform,  one  group  receiving  shock  at  3  minutes  and  another  an 
hour  after  the  training.  The  amnesia  following  shock  was  most  pro- 
nounced in  the  animals  treated  at  9  p.m.  This  surprised  the  experimenters 
who  commented: 

"This  could  mean  either  that  memory  consolidation  varies  inverse- 
ly with  metabolic  activities  or  susceptibility  to  electro  convulsive 
shock  varies  directly  with  metabolic  activity.  Whatever  the  basis, 
such  variations  must  be  considered  in  experimental  investigations  of 
memory  storage  processes." 

The  greatest  response  to  shock  occurred  at  the  time  when  the  animal's 
temperature  reached  its  daily  peak,  at  a  time  when  there  would  be  a  high 
concentration  of  adrenal  steroids  in  the  blood.  A  correspondence  between 
biological  time  of  day  and  the  strength  of  conditoned  fear  and  also  extinc- 
tion has  been  observed  by  Dr.  Charles  Stroebel  of  the  Institute  of  Living 
in  Hartford.  He  postulated  that  certain  emotional  conditioning  might 
show  a  time-lock-iinking  strength  of  memory  and  the  biological  time  of 
day  at  which  the  conditioning  took  place. 

In  a  series  of  experiments  conducted  in  Doctor  McGaugh's  laboratory, 
it  was  found  that  animals  treated  by  day  differed  from  the  animals  treated 
at  night.  Indeed,  animals  who  were  put  into  shock  right  after  multiple 
experiences  on  the  platform  were  unusually  resistant  to  the  amnesic  ef- 
fects of  shock  if  they  had  been  trained  between  9  and  1 1  o'clock  in  the 
evening-their  period  of  high  temperature  and  peak  activity.  This  sug- 
gested again  that  retention  of  avoidance  reactions  must  be  strongly  in- 
fluenced by  physiological  cycles. 

An  animal's  temperature  curve  over  24  hours  is  not  a  sine  wave.  Tem- 
peratures were  obtained  from  72  animals  on  a  schedule  of  1 2  hours  of 
light  and  1 2  hours  of  darkness.  These  animals  were  protected  from  dis- 
turbance and  maintained  on  a  steady,  even  room  temperature.  In  the 
course  of  3  days,  it  was  clear  that  the  rectal  temperature  would  show  a  2* 
to  3°  change  each  24  hours.  The  peak  would  occur  around  9  p.m.,  and  the 
nadir  at  about  1  p.m.  These  were  now  the  hours  selected  for  experimental 
trials. 

In  repeated  trials  on  the  platform,  with  shock  or  sham  shock  delivered 
at  varying  intervals,  it  was  possible  to  examine  the  animals'  rates  of  learn- 
ing. How  quickly  did  they  learn  and  how  resistant  were  they  to  forgetting 
that  they  must  hover  on  that  platform  and  not  enter  the  forbidden  hole? 
In  several  replications  of  the  experiment  at  different  hours  of  day  and 
night,  the  animals'  rates  of  learning  and  persistence  of  memory  were 
scored,  Both  seemed  to  fluctuate,  rising  and  falling  rhythmically  around 

564 


the  clock.  It  suggested  that  the  animals  were  fluctuating  in  some  basic 
level  of  arousal. 

dearly,  retrograde  amnesia  did  not  simply  depend  upon  the  time  al- 
lowed between  the  acquisition  of  some  new  learning  and  an  electro  convul- 
sive shock.  Indeed,  it  depended  upon  the  phase  of  the  animal's  tempera- 
ture rhythms  and  other  factors.  The  most  sizable  fluctuation  seemed  to 
correspond  with  the  daily  rise  and  fall  in  adrenal  steroids  and  temperature. 
Almost  every  system  in  the  body  is  known  to  show  a  roughly  24-hour 
fluctuation  in  function. 

Recently,  the  investigators  have  been  shifting  the  lighting  schedule  3 
hours  in  older  to  see  how  animals  learn  and  remember  during  periods  of 
biological  transition.  If  the  lighting  is  shifted  by  3  hours,  there  are  some 
days  in  which  the  animals  are  readjusting  to  the  new  day-night  cycle,  and 
they  no  longer  show  their  former  crest  of  activity  and  temperature.  Now, 
when  the  experiments  are  run  at  1  p.m.  and  9  p.m.,  the  memory  gradients 
aie  no  longer  the  same.  Perhaps  the  disruption  of  sleep  schedules,  as  this 
experiment  suggests,  also  disrupts  some  memory  functions. 


Implications 

All  of  these  facets  in  the  exploration  of  the  biology  of  memory  are 
bound  to  have  their  impact  upon  education.  Clearly,  the  more  we  under- 
stand about  a  student  and  his  properties  as  a  learner,  the  more  effectively 
he  can  be  taught.  Unfortunately,  today's  classrooms  are  not  run  according 
to  knowledge  about  the  nature  of  memory  and  learning.  Educators  gener- 
ally assume  that  such  information  is  irrelevant  despite  the  fact  that 
memory  is  one  of  the  most  important  aspects  of  a  child's  learning.  One  of 
the  few  contributions  accepted  by  educators  from  psychology  has  been 
the  IQ  test.  This  is  not  used  as  it  should  be,  for  diagnostic  purposes.  This 
test  attempts  to  say  something  about  the  child  as  a  learner  and  to  predict 
something  about  the  child's  capacity  to  learn.  But  the  current  use  of  IQ 
tests  reminds  one  of  a  physician  who  examines  his  patient  and  says.  "You 
have  a  very  bad  liver  and  you  are  likely  to  die  an  early  death.  Very 
interesting,  you  have  3  clays  to  live." 

In  a  very  crude  sense,  the  IQ  test  appears  to  measure  some  adaptability 
that  is  biologically  based.  Many  deficiencies  as  well  as  talents  are  bio- 
logically based.  Some  are  metabolic,  like  phenylketonuria,  which  can  be 
counteracted  by  a  diet  starting  at  birth.  If  it  is  not  corrected,  the  defect 
leads  to  mental  retardation.  Biological  variations  of  normal  intelligence 
and  learning  are  not  presently  analyzed  and  used  for  the  purposes  of 
education,  Where  there  is  strong  evidence  that  family  resemblances  and 
intelligence  have  a  genetic  basis,  general  notions  of  intelligence  are  useless. 
There  are  diverse  reasons  why  a  person  may  be  a  good  or  a  poor  learner. 
For  example,  a  child's  learning  efficiency  depends  on  many  different 
processes.  He  might  be  a  poor  learner  because  he  has  a  deficiency  in 
several  systems  of  storage  and  retrieval.  He  might  have  no  short-term 

565 


memory  or  he  may  not  be  able  to  retrieve  information.  Some  people  have 
good  immediate  memory  but  have  lost  the  ability  to  memorize;  that  is,  to 
learn  any  new  material  and  store  it  for  any  length  of  time. 

Dr.  Breiida  Milner  of  the  Montreal  Neuiological  Institute  has  studied 
memory  in  patients  with  brain  damage  in  both  temporal  lobes.  Often 
these  people  seem  to  be  quite  normal  and  their  IQ  scores  are  unaffected. 
Some  of  them  have  completely  lost  the  ability  to  acquire  and  retain  new 
information.  For  instance,  one  man  had  suffered  brain  damage  10  years 
ago.  While  in  many  respects  he  seems  normal,  his  family  moved  and  he 
never  was  able  to  learn  the  address  of  his  new  house.  He  does  the  same 
jigsaw  puzzles  day  after  day  without  ever  showing  any  effect  from  prac- 
tice. He  reads  the  same  magazine  over  and  over  again,  but  doesn't  find  the 
contents  familiar.  This  type  of  amnesia  has  proved  to  be  very  helpful  in 
understanding  various  aspects  of  memory.  Some  of  the  problems  of  school 
children  may  stem  fiom  kinds  of  memory-retrieval  defects  that  normal 
school  techniques  never  overcome, 

As  Dr.  Millard  Madsen  of  UCLA  has  shown,  high  IQ  and  low  IQ  chil- 
dren can  be  induced  to  learn  at  about  the  same  rate  under  the  appropriate 
conditions.  Undei  the  usual  schoolroom  conditions,  the  children  with  high 
IQ's  seem  to  learn  much  faster.  However,  if  the  low  IQ  children  are 
permitted  a  longer  interval  between  the  introduction  of  new  information, 
they  learn  at  about  the  same  rate  as  the  high  IQ  children.  Thus,  rearrang- 
ing the  school  environment  to  meet  the  individual's  needs  can  eliminate 
the  differences  predicted  by  IQ  scores.  The  role  of  our  present  diagnostic 
tests,  such  as  the  IQ  test,  should  allow  us  to  find  out  what  information- 
storing  process  is  deficient  and  how  to  compensate  for  it. 

The  day  of  the  memory  pill  is  not  quite  here,  but  it  is  coming  soon.  In 
the  future  it  may  be  just  as  common  to  give  Johnny  his  pill  when  he  leaves 
the  house  as  it  is  to  remind  him  to  brush  his  teeth  and  to  put  on  his 
glasses. 

The  relation  between  long-term  memory  and  the  intervals  between 
training,  the  influence  of  shock,  of  drugs,  may  indeed  revolutionize  the 
intellectual  capacity  of  humankind.  We  may  also  learn  how  biological 
rhythms  enhance  or  detract  from  learning  and  may  schedule  learning  for 
the  optimum  time  of  day.  We  may  learn  how  to  encounteract  memory 
deficiencies  with  drugs- thus  opening  the  doors  for  almost  half  of  man- 
kind more  fully  to  realize  their  potential.  Children  may  receive  a  more 
compact  and  rich  education  when  we  begin  to  respond  to  the  way  they 
learn.  For  the  elderly,  too,  the  advent  of  memory  drugs  may  prolong  the 
philosophical  years  of  intellectual  activity  now  curtailed  by  poor  memory. 

Research  Grant:  MH  12526 

Date  of  Interview:  September  1968 


566 


Investigator: 

Seymour  Levine,  Ph.  D. 

Stanford  University  Medical  School 

Stanford,  Calif. 

Prepared  by: 

Gay  Luce 


How  Experience  Shapes  the  Infant 

A  dominant  child-rearing  philosophy  -often  accredited  to  Freud-warns 
that  trauma  and  distress  in  infancy  and  childhood  may  be  the  source  of 
neurosis  and  adult  instability.  As  Americans  have  become  more  affluent, 
increasing  numbers  of  financially  secure  and  devoted  parents  have  ab- 
sorbed the  popular  distillations  of  child-rearing  theoiies,  and  have  been 
striving  to  protect  their  offspring  from  some  of  the  nastier  realities  and 
contretemps  that  they  themselves  encountered  in  early  life.  A  variety  of 
protections  have  been  built  into  the  environment  of  the  middle-class  child. 
The  amount  of  stimulation  in  infancy  has  been  reduced  by  bottles  and 
pacifiers.  In  extreme  designs  for  infant  care  with  minimal  handling  there 
are  thermostatically  controlled  glass  cribs  with  cloth  rollers  that  obviate 
even  the  necessity  of  changing  diapers. 

What  arc  the  consequences  of  increasingly  automated  infant  care  fol- 
lowed by  a  highly  protected  childhood?  There  is  no  way  of  telling  from 
case  histories  in  retrospect.  Instability  and  neurosis  do  not  appear  to  have 
decreased  notably.  Some  observers  have  cited  the  Korean  war  as  evidence 
that  Americans  were  growing  soft,  for  jarring  and  exaggerated  analyses  of 
American  soldiers  had  implied  that  they  capitulated  to  the  enemy  under 
only  moderate  stress.  Some  reporters  thought  these  POW's  had  be  en  i  ren- 
dered unduly  vulnerable  to  stress  by  comfortable  and  protected  back- 
grounds. For  a  time,  the  middle-class  home  was  blamed  for  defections  and 
surrenders  in  Korea  although  they  were  not  statistically  different  trom 
defections  and  surrenders  in  other  wars  and  among  other  groups-Due  rms 

tor  in 


attempt  to  understand  and  explain  behavior  was  not 

retrospect  there  could  be  no  measure  of  the  Pf 

tliat  affected  the  POW's  and  might  have  been  ^m* 

war.   It  was  an  object  lesson  in  the  difficulty  of  dcternann«  ca  uw  an 

effect,  and   might  have  thrown  into  question  the  case  study  basis  tor 

prevailing  theories  of  rearing  the  young. 

567 


Until  the  last  decade  there  was  remarkably  little  scientific  study  of  the 
developmental  process.  There  was  a  small  literature  containing  a  few  con- 
trolled studies  of  human  infants,  observations  by  ethologists,  and  a  few 
neurophysiological  studies.  But  extensive  developmental  research  had  to 
await  an  instrumentation  that  was  not  yet  developed,  and  also  a  change  of 
attitudes,  A  good  many  explorations  could  not  be  conducted  upon  human 
infants;  the  physiological  effects  of  infant  experience  had  to  be  learned  in 
laboratory  experiments  on  animals.  Initially,  these  studies  of  infant  experi- 
ence made  the  presumption  that  the  most  important,  most  scarring  effects 
might  be  blamed  on  trauma-which  was  roughly  assumed  to  be  equivalent 
to  any  intense  and  unpleasant  experience.  Scientists  searched  for  the  ef- 
fects of  trauma. 

Many  early  attempts  to  find  the  effects  of  trauma  in  infancy  were  often 
conducted  with  mice  or  rats,  but  the  methodology  left  the  question  of 
effects  unanswered.  During  the  1950's  Dr.  Levine  and  his  various  col- 
leagues began  to  compare  groups  of  infant  animals:  one  group  would  be 
subjected  to  handling,  shock,  and  other  treatments  while  the  control 
group  was  strictly  exempt  from  all  treatment  and  handling.  Comparisons 
of  these  groups  clearly  suggested  that  even  a  seemingly  mild  experience 
would  leave  perceptible  traces  in  the  animal's  behavior  and  physiology. 

In  some  studies  the  infant  mice  or  rats  were  shaken  in  bottles,  shocked, 
or  vibrated  in  their  cages.  The  investigators  expected  their  tests  to  show 
that  these  manhandled  infants  were  neurotic  and  emotional  while  the 
controls  behaved  properly  and  matured  faster.  The  effect  seemed  to  occur 
in  reverse.  Rats  handled  in  infancy  might  be  placed  in  a  large  open  con- 
tainer as  adults  and  would  show  little  timidity,  They  would  rummage 
about-white  the  controls  cowered  in  corners.  It  seemed,  indeed,  that  tlie 
handled  animal  was  less  emotional. 

One  measure  of  distress  and  emotionality  in  rodents  has  been  the  open 
field  test.  When  taken  from  a  cage  and  placed  in  a  larger  barren  container, 
the  emotional  and  distressed  rat  will  tend  to  crouch  and  cower  in  one  spot 
rather  than  wander  around.  He  will  also  defecate  and  urinate  a  great  deal, 
The  calmer  animal  will  eliminate  much  less  and  will  explore  his  new 
setting.  Involuntary  elimination  under  fear  and  stress  has  provided  a  good 
many  battle  jokes,  for  it  has  been  a  relatively  common  experience  among 
fighting  men,  It  is  one  of  the  responses  of  the  sympathetic  nervous  system 
when  activated  by  the  neurochemical  network  that  functions  during 
stress.  The  indices  of  nervous  elimination  and  activity  in  open  field  tests 
have  been  useful  measures  of  behavioral-physiological  differences  among 
rodents. 

Another  criterion  used  to  judge  a  rat's  adaptive  behavior  is  avoidance 
conditioning,  a  procedure  in  which  the  animal  must  press  a  bar  or  perform 
some  action  in  order  to  escape  being  shocked,  A  traumatized  animal  might 
be  expected  to  learn  avoidance  very  slowly.  With  this  expectation,  an 
early  experiment  divided  the  infant  rats  into  three  groups.  One  group  was 
shocked,  one  group  was  put  through  all  the  motions  preparatory  to  shock 
but  not  the  shock  itself,  and  the  third  group  was  left  to  rest  quietly  in 
Isolated  cages-never  handled  at  all,  The  shock  was  evidently  painful 

568 


enough  that  the  infant  rats  squeaked  and  tried  to  escape,  and  the  experi- 
menters expected  that  these  creatures  might  later  show  signs  of  trauma. 
When  they  became  mature  they  were  placed  in  an  avoidance  cage.  The 
previously  shocked  animals  learned  to  avoid  shock  most  rapidly,  with  the 
other  handled  animals  pel  forming  nearly  as  well.  However,  when  the 
totally  untieated  animals  were  put  to  the  test  they  did  not  react  normally 
at  all.  Instead  of  running  and  jumping  when  shocked  they  would  tend  to 
freeze  and  defecate.  Their  lack  of  infantile  experience  had  apparently  left 
them  incapable  of  confronting  a  new  situation,  and  they  were  so  flustered 
that  they  showed  what  might  be  called,  in  human  terms,  the  signs  of  a 
paralyzing  fear. 

Infant  experience,  even  if  quite  noxious,  seemed  to  gear  the  animal  to 
meet  new  situations  and  stress  in  adulthood,  enhancing  his  capacity  to 
survive  in  a  changing  environment.  As  subsequent  experiments  indicated, 
lack  of  infant  experience  was  indeed  a  severe  handicap  in  the  survival 
behavior  of  the  rat.  Even  when  quite  thirsty  the  imhandled  rats  would 
drink  little  in  a  new  situation.  When  shocked  they  became  so  flustered 
they  drank  less  and  less.  Here  again,  the  rats  had  been  divided  into  three 
groups.  All  were  deprived  of  water  for  nearly  a  day.  Then  they  were  given 
water  and  the  amount  they  drank  was  measured.  The  untreated  rats  drank 
least. 

Latei ,  when  the  thirsty  rats  were  shocked  before  receiving  water,  all  of 
the  groups  drank  less,  but  as  this  procedure  continued  the  untreated  group 
drank  least  of  all.  Throughout  the  study  this  naive  group  persisted  in 
drinking  very  little,  whereas  the  shocked  and  handled  groups  apparently 
got  used  to  the  situation  and  began  drinking  more.  The  creature  with  an 
infancy  empty  of  varying  experience  appeared  so  overcome  when  con- 
fronted with  stress  that  it  would  not  even  drink  when  very  thirsty. 

Further  experiments  began  to  suggest  that  the  inexperienced  animal 
might  react  poorly  to  an  aversive  test,  but  if  it  were  merely  novel,  the 
animals  would  perform  more  adequately  as  they  grew  accustomed  to  it. 
They  simply  took  much  longer  to  adjust  than  did  the  handled  creatures. 
A  deeper  exploration  of  the  animals's  reaction  to  shock  indicated  that 
the  handled  animals  would  learn  avoidance  faster  at  low  levels  of  shock. 
Although  these  creatures  were  supposed  to  be  less  emotional,  they  gave 
signs  of  being  more  responsive  than  the  untreated  creatures  to  acute 
shock.  Infantile  stimulation  did  not  merely  make  an  animal  less  emo- 
tional: apparently  it  enabled  him  to  make  discriminations  about  relevant 
attributes  of  the  environment  and  adjust  his  responses  accordingly. 

By  what  physiological  process  was  this  experience  altering  the  emo- 
tional reactivity  and  learning  ability  to  the  animals?  The  effects  of  infan- 
tile stimulation  were  explored  by  Dr.  Levine  and  others  in  a  multitude  of 
experiments.  They  soon  found  themselves  examining  the  endocrine  sys- 
tem which  suffuses  the  animal  with  emergency  chemicals  during  stress. 
One  measure  of  stress  in  an  animal  is  the  level  of  certain  adrenal  steroids 

in  the  blood. 

When   crisis  occurs  and  an  animal  is  threatened,  the  central  nervous 
system  transmits  the  alarm  to  two  important  and  interconnected  glandular 

569 


systems.  Messages  from  the  brain  will  set  into  action  the  pituitary,  an 
inconspicuous  egg-shaped  gland  which  in  man  resides  just  above  the  nasal 
passages,  at  the  base  of  the  brain.  The  pituitary  controls  quite  a  few 
specialized  sex  hormones,  exerts  influence  upon  the  thyroid  gland,  con- 
tiols  growth  hormones.  On  occasions  of  stress  it  releases  ACTH,  adreno- 
corticotropic  hormone,  which  mobilizes  the  adrenal  glands  far  distant 
The  adienals,  two  small  yellowish  capsules  above  the  kidneys,  respond  to 
ACTH  by  releasing  several  hormones  into  the  system.  One  of  these  is  r?ie 
familiar  stimulant  adrenalin,  whose  action  increases  the  available  blood 
sugar.  When  a  stress  outpouring  continues  for  some  time,  the  adrenal 
glands  will  enlarge.  Thus  a  good  deal  can  be  learned  about  an  animal's 
intensity  of  stress  reaction  by  measuring  adrenal  steroids  in  the  blood  or 
by  weighing  the  adrenal  glands  themselves,  and  analyzing  their  content. 
These  measures  indeed  may  be  used  to  predict  how  an  animal  will  behave, 
although  not  invariably,  for  many  genetic  factors  must  be  taken  into 
account. 

As  Dr,  Levine  and  his  associates  probed  the  consequences  of  infant 
handling,  they  found  that  a  detectable  and  sometimes  exceedingly  largo 
physiological  change  occurs  and  persists  into  adulthood.  In  lecent  studies 
they  have  been  examining  the  differences  between  handled  and  untreated 
rais  after  shock-now  by  a  combination  of  behavioral  and  physical  tests. 

Manipulated  infant  rats  and  untouched  controls  were  subjected  to 
shock:  during  the  first  15  minutes  after  shock  it  was  clear  that  the 
handled  animals  produced  more  adrenal  steroids.  At  first  it  looked  as  if 
handling  had  made  the  animal  overreactive,  but  in  fact  it  simply  caused 
him  to  respond  fast.  When  shocked,  he  would  summon  his  adrenal  forces 

rapidly,  receiving  a  rich  suffusion  of  emergency  steroids  for  the  period  of 
stress,  bvit  the  steroid  level  would  subside  quickly  buck  to  normal.  The 
unhandled  rats,  by  contrast,  were  reacting  slowly  to  stress.  They  con- 
tinued pouring  emergency  fuel  into  their  systems  long  after  the  crisis  had 
passed,  They  might  be  like  the  peison  who  awakens  slowly  to  disaster  and 
remains  perturbed  and  anxious  long  after  it  has  passed.  They  are  not 
geared  to  act  promptly  but  endure  a  prolonged  aftermath  of  stress  physi- 
ology which  might  indeed  incur  damages,  perhaps  causing  ulcers  or  suscep- 
tibility to  infection, 

Handling  in  infancy,  as  a  long  program  of  studies  began  to  show, 
improved  the  stress  physiology  in  the  maturing  animal  and  adult.  The 
investigators  began  to  find  that  the  handled  animals  matured  far  more 
rapidly  than  the  unhandled  controls.  There  were  many  indices  for  the 
maturation  rate.  Ordinarily  the  pituitary  gland  of  the  laboratory  rat  docs 
not  release  ACTH  until  the  animal  is  about  16  clays  old.  Handled  rats, 
however,  were  producing  and  releasing  ACTH  by  about  1 2  days. 

The  brains  differed.  As  the  infant  brain  matures,  all  the  nerve  fibers  in 
the  brain  and  body  that  link  neural  cells  become  ensheathed  in  a  fatty 
substance,  myelin.  This  white  matter,  as  it  is  often  called,  is  largely  com- 
posed of  cholesterol.  When  the  brains  of  handled  infant  rats  were  com- 
pared with  those  of  the  unmanipulated  controls,  the  handled  rats  showed 

570 


considerably  more  cholesterol,  perhaps  indicating  that  myelinization  was 
occurring  faster. 

Experiments  with  deliberately  induced  brain  damage  again  pointed  up 
the  difference  between  the  handled  and  unhandled  animals,  Lesions 
within  the  septal  region  of  the  hippocampus  cause  hyperexcitability  and 
even  viciousness.  After  lesions  were  made,  however,  the  handled  animals 
did  not  become  as  excitable  and  certainly  not  as  vicious  as  did  the  un- 
handled controls.  These  brain-damaged  innocents,  the  experimenters 
noted,  were  the  most  vicious  little  i  orients  they  had  ever  seen  and  would 
pursue  people  around  the  laboiatory,  attacking  their  ankles  and  legs. 

The  overt  maturation  rate  of  the  handled  infants  was  well  in  advance  of 
the   unhandled   controls.   They   simply   grew   faster,   opened  their  eyes 
sooner,  moved  in  a  coordinated  fashion  earlier,  developing  a  good  coat  of 
fur  and  gaining  weight  rapidly.  Since  these  handled  rats  did  not  appear  to 
be  eating  more  than  their  naive  controls,  it  was  speculated  that  they  might 
have  developed  a  more  efficient  protein  metabolism.  The  handled  group 
survived   the  longest.  The  infant  who  had  been  moved  around,  joggled, 
shocked,  and  subjected  to  various  stresses  was  clearly  better  equipped  to 
survive  than  the  creature  who  had  been  raised  in  a  completely  sterile 
fashion. 

There  is,  of  course,  a  huge  difference  between  the  laboratory  environ- 
ment of  the  piotected  animal  and  the  environment  he  would  enjoy  living 
in  a  cage  with  his  mother  and  littermates  or,  surely,  in  the  natural  environ- 
ment. His  is  a  world  composed  of  a  temperature-controlled  cage,  aseptic 
and  barren,  with  light  and  sound  artificially  regulated.  Thus,  the  labor- 
atory study  of  infant  experience  compares  animals  subjected  to  extreme 
and  purified  conditions.  The  experimenters  have  consequently  been  very 
cautious  about  generalizing  their  findings  to  human  beings.  What  they 
Jiave  indicated,  in  their  purification  of  environments,  is  that  some  range  of 
stimulation  even  if  noxious  must  be  necessary  to  the  normal  maturation 
of  a  mammal  and  will  leave  its  imprint  upon  the  physiology  and  behavior 
of  the  adult  animal.  Ordinarily  neither  animals  nor  infant  humans  pass 
their  earliest  months  in  an  environmental  vacuum.  Perhaps  the  closest 
approximation  to  a  vacuum  in  the  life  of  the  human  baby  is  that  of  the 
orphanage.  Orphanage  babies  are  rarely  picked  upon,  spoken  to,  played 
with,  and  moved  around. 

Clinical  studies  of  orphans  have  indicated  that  they  were  often  retarded 
in  development,  timid,  unable  to  adapt  to  changing  situations,  and  indeed 
more  susceptible  to  disease  than  the  home  child  of  the  same  age.  Some 
investigators  attributed  the  debilities  of  the  orphan  to  the  (ack  of  a 
mother.  Today,  however,  animal  studies  make  it  possible  to  offer  another 
conjecture.  It  is  the  mother  who  lifts  and  handles  a  child,  speaking  to  it, 
Teeding,  playing,  and  expending  upon  it  a  prodigious  amount  of  "atten- 
tion." Perhaps  the  missing  element  in  the  orphan's  development  is  the 
motion,,  the  stimulation  ordinarily  provided  by  a  devoted  parent.  Very 
recently,  in  the  first  controlled  study  of  its  kind,  White  and  Held  have 
sliown  that  aspects  of  visual  acuity  and  adaptation  in  very  young  infants 
in  orphanages  can  be  "speeded  up"  by  enriching  the  visual  environments 

571 


of  the  crib.  Only  longitudinal  studies  will  tell  whether  the  infant  in  his 
usual  orphanage  environment  wiU  later  catch  up  with  the  experimental 
infants  and  whether,  indeed,  effects  of  early  visual  experience  can  be 
detected  in  future  behavior. 

In  the  animal  studies  of  Dr.  Levine  and  his  associates,  employing  a 
purified  situation,  it  has  become  increasingly  clear  that  manipulation 
during  infancy  has  produced  more  responsive  adult  animals.  Rats  that 
were  left  strictly  alone  in  infancy  were  shocked  as  adults:  they  took 
considerable  amounts  of  shock  before  they  would  learn  to  avoid  it.  Their 
counterparts,  handled  during  infancy,  required  little  shock  to  teach  them 
avoidance.  These  handled  rats  appeared  to  be  making  more  sensitive  discri- 
minations about  events  impinging  on  them. 

The  experimenters  predicted  that  early  handling  would  help  the  cre- 
ature respond  to  new  situations,  and  one  measure  of  response  would  be 
the  amount  of  stress  registered  physically-the  output  of  steroids.  Rats  at 
the  weaning  stage  weie  set  down  in  a  new  cage  for  a  few  minutes  and  then 
their  steroids  weie  measured,  The  handled  rats  showed  n  much  lower 
output  of  stress  steroids  than  did  the  nonhandled  controls.  Laboratory 
tests  of  this  kind  may  seem  remote  from  their  human  analogy-in  part 
because  we  have  precious  little  empathy  for  rats.  It  may  be  possible  to 
imagine  the  experience  of  the  infant  rat  by  recalling  the  reactions  of  a 
small  infant.  He  is  accustomed  to  sleeping  in  his  own  crib  and  he  is 
suddenly  tiansported  to  a  new  house  where  he  is  left  upon  a  large  open 
bed.  Some  infants  will  cry  and  fuss  showing  signs  of  fear  and  distress.  The 
first  time  an  infant  is  lifted  up  and  held  by  slrangeis  he  may  produce  a 
fearful  wail.  Surely,  the  infant  human  shows  signs  of  intense  distress  when 
fust  exposed  to  a  night  in  a  hospital,  and  many  parents  comment  that 
their  babies  are  generally  resistant  to  change.  One  may  imagine  that  the 
laboratory  rat  endures  drastic  experiences  when  he  is  first  picked  up  by  a 
giant,  removed  from  his  familiar  cage,  and  set  down  in  a  new  place  with 
unfamiliar  smells.  The  first  time  he  con  ft  on  ts  the  open-field  test  he  must 
experience  some  of  the  emotional  upheavals  that  might  occur  in  an  infant 
who  is  lifted  from  his  crib  and  set  into  a  gigantic  baby-pen  the  size  of  a 
huge  loom. 

Both  handled  and  unhandled  rats  reacted  similarly  to  their  first  open- 
field  test,  if  judged  by  their  movements  around  the  container  and  amount 
of  elimination,  But  when  the  experience  was  repeated,  the  effects  of 
handling  were  apparent.  The  handled  rats  now  acted  more  "at  home"  and 
explored  more  and  eliminated  less.  The  "protected"  rats,  on  the  contrary, 
acted  more  emotional  than  before.  They  reduced  their  activity  and  defe- 
cated more.  They  did  not  seem  to  have  benefited  from  their  first  experi- 
ence but  acted  as  if  this  were  a  new  and  even  more  threatening  situation. 

The  effects  of  infant  handling  have  been  studied  in  other  species  than 
the  rat.  The  monkey  is,  of  course,  much  closer  to  man  phylogentically, 
and  quite  a  number  of  investigators  have  been  looking  with  interest  at  the 
effects  of  early  experience  on  monkeys.  Recently,  in  collaboration  with 
Dr.  William  Mason  of  the  Delta  Primate  Center,  Dr.  Levine  and  his  col- 
leagues have  been  able  to  follow  the  behavioral  and  physiological  effects 

572 


of  handling  in  infant  monkeys.  They  have  recently  compared  the  stress 
and  learning  responses  of  three  pairs  of  monkeys.  One  pail  was  raised  with 
only  a  stationary  dummy  for  a  mother.  Another  pair  had  a  robot  mother 
that  was  motorized  and  capable  of  certain  motions.  The  third  pair  had 
been  laised  natuially  by  their  mothers  in  the  wild.  When  the  six  animals 
anived  at  the  Stanford  laboratory  they  bore  no  identification  tags  saying 
which  was  which.  Their  behavioial  differences  were  so  great,  however, 
that  it  was  quickly  apparent.  One  of  the  fhst  examinations  required  plac- 
ing each  monkey  in  a  restraining  chair  while  blood  samples  were  drawn 
and  steroid  levels  analyzed.  The  experimenteis  wanted  to  determine  some- 
thing about  the  baseline  responses  of  the  animals  to  ordinary  laboratory 
handling  and  to  get  some  reactions  to  very  minoi  and  standard  stimula- 
tion. 

When  seated  in  the  restraining  chair,  a  small  blood  sample  was  taken 
from  the  leg,  A  half  hour  later,  electrodes  were  applied  so  that  galvanic 
skin  responses  could  be  obtained  as  tones  were  played.  Another  blood 
sample  was  drawn  at  this  time.  The  motherless  monkeys  gave  a  low- 
galvanic  skin  response  and  at  first  their  steroid  levels  were  also  very  low. 
As  the  procedure  continued,  the  steroid  response  rose  high  above  the 
others.  When  this  procedure  was  repeated,  the  reactions  of  the  feral  and 
motherless  monkeys  were  more  nearly  the  same.  The  experimenters  began 
to  look  at  behavior. 

Now  the  animals  were  placed  in  a  choice  situation.  They  were  presented 
with  two  different  visual  patterns  on  two  panels.  When  the  monkey 
pushed  the  right  panel  he  automatically  received  a  peanut.  The  feral 
monkeys,  once  they  mastered  the  notion  of  pressing  panels,  learned  the 
correct  discrimination  in  few  trials.  The  monkeys  raised  with  robot 
mothers  took  more  practice  but  learned  the  problem  in  far  less  time  than 
did  the  monkeys  raised  with  stationary  dummies. 

When  the  monkeys  were  transfeired  to  a  new  discrimination  apparatus, 
the  differences  again  became  evident.  The  wild  monkeys  learned  with  their 
former  speed.  The  monkeys  mothered  by  robots  learned,  also.  But  the 
monkeys  raised  on  stationary  dummies  never  learned.  Now  the  problem 
was  reversed  so  that  the  formerly  wrong  choice  was  correct  and  rewarded. 
This  taxed  the  adaptiveness  and  resilience  of  the  monkeys.  On  the  first 
day  of  reversal  there  was  no  reinforcement.  The  feral  and  robot-raised 
monkeys  continued  responding  rapidly  as  before.  On  the  next  day,  when 
reward  was  offered  for  the  formerly  wrong  choice,  the  feral  monkeys 
adapted  with  no  show  of  difficulty.  The  robot-raised  monkeys  took  twice 
as  long,  behaving  as  if  this  were  a  totally  new  situation.  The  monkeys 
raised  on  stationary  dummies  never  did  learn.  The  experimenters  observed 
that  the  maternally  deprived  monkeys  reacted  to  each  change,  each  varia- 
tion in  a  situation  as  if  it  were  now  totally  novel  and  inimical.  Character- 
istically, steroid  samples  indicated  that  these  deprived  monkeys  were  both 
sluggish  in  responding  and  then  overreactive. 

An  initial  look  at  these  three  pairs  of  monkeys  suggested  that  infant 
experience  might  have  far-reaching  and  subtle  effects  upon  the  animal's 
later  ability  to  learn,  to  generalize  from  one  situation  to  its  variant.  From 

573 


the  very  fiist  it  was  clear  that  novelty  of  any  kind  presented  the  mother- 
less monkeys  with  a  painful  problem  of  adjustment.  They  would  crouch 
and  cower  and  scream  when  placed  in  the  learning  cage-as  if  learning  to 
push  a  panel  were  a  wretched  torture.  When  they  finally  mastered  a  discri- 
mination between  two  panels  and  were  asked  to  make  the  reverse  choice 
among  the  alternatives,  they  acted  as  if  they  had  been  dropped  into  an 
entirely  new  situation. 

These  maternally  deprived  animals  have,  of  course,  lacked  more  than 
just  the  handling,  training,  and  affection  that  their  mothers  would  have 
bestowed  during  critical  peiiods.  They  lacked,  as  well,  whatevei  bio- 
chemical modulation  they  might  have  received  through  their  mother's 
milk.  A  program  of  controlled  studies  with  monkeys,  beginning  in  1967, 
may  begin  to  illuminate  the  weight  of  these  many  factors  in  mature  ad- 
justment. These  studies  may  also  begin  to  explain  why  the  inexperienced 
infant  fails  to  become  adept  at  making  discriminations  later.  Is  h&  handi- 
capped by  a  physiological  system  that  is  slow  to  react  to  a  stimulus  and 
then  overreactive?  Is  he  thus  placed  in  a  state  of  anxiety,  feeling  so  con- 
tinually overwrought  that  he  cannot  take  note  of  the  subtle  distinctions  in 
his  surroundings  and  must  react  to  everything? 

In  a  decade  of  animal  studies,  the  investigator  has  consistently  observed 
that  infant  handling  influences  later  behavior  by  adjusting  the  animal's 
level  of  physiological  response  to  stress,  What  is  the  physiological  mecha- 
nism through  which  cxpei  fence  regulates  the  stress  sensitivity  of  the  mature 
animal?  Why  does  early  handling  instill  responses  to  "crisis"  that  are 
prompt  and  brief?  Why  does  lack  of  infant  experience  create  an  adrenal 
response  that  is  sluggish  and  disproportionate? 


Adrenal  Hormones  and  Experience 

Early  studies  of  internal  responses  were  often,  of  necessity,  acute,  and 
the  animal  had  to  be  killed  to  find  the  physical  effects.  Determinations  of 
adrenal  responses  were  made  by  examining  the  glands  themselves  and  by 
measuring  steroid  levels  in  sizable  quantities  of  blood.  More  recently  the 
experimenters  have  devised  a  method  of  determining  steroid  levels  in  very 
small  amounts  of  tissue,  in  a  drop  of  blood.  Very  precise  determinations 
can  be  made  on  one  drop  of  blood  by  fluorescence  measurement,  follow- 
ing this  new  method— a  distinct  advantage  when  several  measures  are  re- 
quired and  the  experimental  animal  is  a  small  rat  or  mouse.  Using  this 
technique,  it  has  been  possible  to  follow  the  steroid  responses  in  handled 
and  nonhandled  animals  over  repeated  tests  without  harming  the  animal, 
Now  it  is  possible  to  obtain  frequent  steroid  determinations  during  stimu- 
lation and  behavior. 

Since  adrenal  steroids  seem  to  correspond  to  a  level  "emotionality"  or 
reactivity,  the  experimenters  have  inevitably  asked  what  role  these  ste- 
roids play  in  behavior.  Early  studies  suggested  that  steroids  appeared  to 
alert  the  animal  for  emergency  action.  When  the  steroid  levels  were  exces- 
sively high,  however,  the  animal  appeared  to  be  too  keyed  up  to  make 

574 


cnsitivc  discriminations.  OIIL-  ol  UK*  IMM  whulw  In,  olwrving  the  effect 
if  steroid  Icvols  on  bchnvior  was  llu;  HVOU.IIIU.  situation.  Hats  plnccd I  in  ,1 
mall  cage  or  maze  with  «u  <'kvtid.nl  Inor  |intl  would  burn  quile  a 
aricty  of  behaviors  to  avoid  ^'H^',  Nli»i'UI.  In  a  recent  Viinanl  on  this 
voidance  conditioning,  rats  h:ivc  h;uJ  lo  IIMIII  a  U-mpomi  rhythm.  Jlns 
as  allowed  the  experimenters  to  IIUMSHIO  di(inr.i's  in  holmvior  wilhprcai 
iccision. 

When  shocks  wore  piicx-d  to  anivr  i-\viy  ,M)  wimls,  the  animal  luitl  lo 
rcss  a  bar  before  the  shock  in  ordn  tt>  pn-voni  il,  IK>  would  quickly  learn 
int  he  did  not  need  to  press  tin-  h.ir  <  uniinuothly.  lie  would  develop  a 
iyUim>  pi  ess  ing  about  every  I  H  MI  P>  swimls.  l;adi  day.  when  he  was 
rat  placed  in  (he  nvoidnnci'  vluimlu-i,  Mir  iiniiuul  would  receive  n  few 
ocks  as  he  rcailjustod  to  tin-  apjMr.itus.  1ml  his  pi-rlurinmict'  would  soon 
icoinc  steady,  his  rhyllnn  aci'tii-id',  (Jivvii  .in  injct'liiui  ol'  ACTII,  then 
ion  his  performance  was  alivjitly  M.iMi'.  hi*  \vniild  hecoine  even  more 
ficicnt.  When  placed  in  tin-  I'hmnhrr.  hr  wunl<l  receive  fewer  .shocks 
ring  the  warimip  puruxl.  Ik*  »|MI  niailc  II'WIT  u^pniiiics,  more  nminiloly 
nctl.  The  priming  sloroid  ;ip|H%tu'il  l<t  fiih.uuv  his  precision.  When  the 
.  was  injected  with  an  uvi-n  IIM»JC  |n»ln»(  Nlerniif,  he  would  hccoinc 
rceptibly  more  ufficionl.  "I  hi'  sli'mul  u-riiii-d  lo  he  enlminrinB  liienlcrt- 
^s  of  the  animal. 

In  the  noimnl  course  ol'evcnls.  llu-  .uhoii.tl  sUMoiiKsiifntse  Ihchodyby 
:hain  of  reactions.  A  stiinultis  WOM'V  Ihr  IU.IHI  to  sentl  .slgnuls  lo  Ilio 
uilary,  which  relenses  A<vni,  ihtiMJiiwnn  Die  iulronids  to  release  their 
milnting  chemicuis  into  the  MomMiiMMi  ,iml  nc«r  the  hotly  for  action. 
.1  immcduiio  supply   of  hlo.t.1  -.U}.',ii  h  tiu reused  hy  tt  hrcakdown^ol 
red  glycogen,  giving    I  In-    huily  iHMr.t  eiternv  lo  use.  Hut  I  lie  alerting 
jcls,  the  potent  inlluoiKV  iipini  In'h.iviur0  must  come  from  (he  effects 
ilcroids  upon  the  ccnlnil  ncrAHi^  ivsinn  itwll  OneMepin  nscorlniinng 
v  these  steroid  hormones  inlpht  %h.ipr  nc«r,il  rtsptmscs,  1ms  been  lo 
eh  hehavior  when  sterofils  ;m<  ;ul*uirii^m-tl  tliratly  lo  the  brain-  Poos 
hrain  rcspontl  to  those  sleiouNh| 

)r.  Luvino  and  his  nssociates  IIAU*  twyun  ;i  H-rks  of  experiments ^  in 
i'h  they  are  imphmlin^  Mcrnhh  thuvily  iniu  ilw  brnln  llirou.gh.t!|i; 
ow  cannulae.  In  otTecl,  tliis  V.IIMC"I  (lit*  hniin  tu  "believe"  t««  r"F 
>tcl  level  of  the  hotly  is  a  grcar  ik^il  liiplicr  than  il  bHiiactwaliiy-  U  : so. 
t'xporhncnlcrs  have  postviluUxl  lh«l  ihc  animal  should  be  exceedingly 

nml  learn  to  avoid   punishnu'ni  my  *wil'rly.  When  hydrocortison^ 
ol  the  adrenal  steroids,  lins  hern  nurclcd  Into  the  hypotlinlmnus  01 » 
ulhcr  no  lon«er  onuses  a  strc«\  rc^ptinw  Mwcowr.wJionihB*™"1;" 
.'il  iii  a  sluittlchox.  il  will  (turn  to  jmtd  shock  very  quickly  n"«  *J 
.'uver  to  escape  being  shocked  •!*  Hmc*  out  of  60.  A  control  ™  j 
w  hrain  has  received  instead  «»r  «lcroid»n  implant  of  cholesterol,  w 
I  this  .same  shock  much  les*  ^irkkntly  and  Hwrofore  rece  w  nw 
^  in  tlie  shutdchox.  U  would  .ipp<r«r  Hint  (Jie  adrenal  steroj cs^ 
inlluonco  upon  the  brain,  lfu>,  enhandng  «ho  abfHiy  10 dlscnmin 
oulance  situations  situations  in  which  the  stakes  must  be  n«M 

575 


the  animal's  point  of  view,  for  the  failure  to  respond  means  a  very  unplea- 
sant consequence. 

These  studies  of  adrenal  steroids,  as  they  directly  influence  the  cental 
nervous  system,  are  filling  in  the  puzzle  of  infant  handling  and  its  conse- 
quences in  adulthood.  Handling,  a  wide  variety  of  stimulations,  and  in- 
deed stresses,  appeals  to  alter  a  basic  hormonal  system  during  infancy, 
Without  handling  and  stress,  the  infant  and  the  adult  animal  learns  avoid- 
ance slowly,  does  not  discriminate  readily,  and  exhibits  disproportionate 
emotionality  when  placed  in  a  novel  situation  for  the  first  time.  He  exhi- 
bits a  slow  but  inordinate  level  of  adienal  steroids  whenever  confronted 
with  a  challenge  or  shock.  His  excessive  output  after  repeated  stiess  is 
reflected  in  adrenal  glands  that  are  unusually  large  and  heavy,  by  stunted 
growth,  by  susceptibility  to  disease.  How  does  experience  create  the  dif- 
ferent and  healthier  pattern  of  the  handled  infant? 

The  experimenters  have  postulated  that  handling  during  critical  periods 
of  infancy  must  allow  the  system  to  adjust  its  "stress"  system  by  a  series 
of  approximations.  Perhaps  the  system  operates  in  a  manner  analogous  to 
a  thermostat.  The  animal  reacts  to  an  event  and  pours  out  adrenal  steroids 
by  which  the  brain  responds  according  to  their  level,  thus  regulating 
further  behavior.  The  shaping  that  occurs  is  in  the  organization  of  emo- 
tional responses  via  the  pattern  of  released  corticosleroids,  a  pattern  thai 
is  thought  to  be  controlled  by  the  hypothalamus.  Release  of  these  fear- 
inspired  steroids  may  have  a  permanent  effect  upon  the  hypothalamus 
during  critical  periods  of  infancy,  thus  calibrating  the  animal's  level  of 
response  to  stimuli.  Without  experience,  there  can  be  no  comparison, 
none  of  the  raw  material  by  which  the  central  nervous  system  could 
establish  a  ratio  of  response  and  event-setting  up  an  internal  sense  of 
proportion.  If  the  nervous  system  of  the  experienced  infant  is  calibrated 
so  that  responses  are  rapid,  brief,  and  create  little  aftermath-cues  to  this 
development  must  lie  within  the  steroid  levels  of  the  newborn  and  infant 
animal. 

A  number  of  factors-among  them  size  and  cost -have  made  rodents  the 
most  convenient  animals  for  many  of  these  studies.  Different  strains  of 
inbred  rats  and  mice  show  distinctive  characteristics  in  their  stress  re- 
sponses, their  normal  excitability,  and  their  responses  to  handling.  When 
laboratory  animals  are  purchased,  moreover,  it  is  hard  to  determine 
exactly  how  the  infants  have  been  handled  by  the  breeder  and,  therefore, 
how  much  of  their  response  pattern  is  genetic,  how  much  due  to  experi- 
ence. Genetic  differences  exert  a  profound  influence  upon  the  develop- 
ment of  the  individual  animal  as  he  interacts  with  his  environment.  For 
this  reason,  the  laboratory  has  engaged  in  a  number  of  genetic  studies  and 
has  compared  quite  a  few  strains  of  rodents  on  a  number  of  criteria.  There 
have  been  studies  of  differences  in  adult  emotionality,  studies  of  the 
effects  of  infant  handling,  and  of  the  adrenal  output  of  various  strains  of 
mice  under  stress.  These  genetic  studies  add  some  important  dimensions 
to  the  study  of  infant  development. 

576 


Genetic  Differences  in  Hormonal  Predisposition 

No  creature  is  completely  plastic  at  birth.  The  shape  of  the  body,  the 
color  of  its  fur,  the  arrangement  of  vital  organs,  and  many  other  internal 
patterns  are  clearly  laid  out  by  the  genes.  Many  behaviors  are  determined 
by  the  structure  of  the  nervous  system,  and  neural  circuitry  is  preset.  The 
nervous  system  contains  a  network  of  communication  fibers  determining 
which  cells  shall  receive  and  transmit  impulses  to  other  particular  cells. 
These  nerve  circuits  determine  how  cells  in  the  eye  shall  transmit  to  the 
visual  brain  thus  structuring  the  animal's  vision.  Even  relatively  specific 
predilections  such  as  preferred  kinds  of  food  may  be  determined  before 
birth.  Early  studies  have  indicated  that  experience  in  infancy  may  perform 
the  next  modification,  adjusting  factors  that  control  the  readiness  of  an 
emergency  response,  the  extent  of  a  reaction,  and  the  length  of  time  that 
an  animal  is  alerted,  his  intensity.  Here,  too,  the  animal  has  some  genetic 
predilections.  Steroid  measurements  are  being  used  to  distinguish  some  of 
the  basic  differences  which  appear  in  the  magnitude  of  the  animal's  re- 
actions to  handling.  Some  strains  of  mouse,  for  example,  exhibit  a  hyper- 
reactiveness  and  a  strong  steroid  response  to  stress.  These  animals  show 
great  differenes  in  the  production  and  release  of  steroids  following  shocks. 
The  reactive  mice,  placed  in  an  open-field  test,  exhibit  a  notable  tendency 
to  cower  in  corners,  to  defecate.  The  others  show  less  emotionality. 

The  genetic  foundations  of  a  response  pattern  are  important  if  the 
effects  of  experimental  manipulation  are  to  be  measured  precisely.  In 
working  with  mice  it  has  been  advisable  to  explore  this  baseline  of  respon- 
siveness and  a  number  of  studies  have  been  undertaken  recently.  Wild 
mice,  as  compared  with  domestic  mice,  show  differences  that  may  tell  a 
good  deal  about  the  way  in  which  the  creature  becomes  tailored  to  the 
particular  demands  of  his  environment.  Wild  mice,  for  instance,  show  a 
much  larger  steroid  response  to  laboratory  tests,  and  have,  indeed,  larger 
adrenals  than  domestic  mice. 

In  a  current  study,  genetic  differences  showed  up  when  the  creatures 
were  presented  with  a  conflict  situation.  The  mice  were  given  no  water  to 
drink  in  their  cages.  They  were  placed  in  a  special  cage  where  they  had  to 
drink  from  a  water  bottle.  After  5  days  they  received  a  shock  while 
drinking  from  the  bottle.  The  strain  with  a  high-steroid  response  quit 
drinking  after  5  shocks,  but  the  low-steroid  strain  took  15  shocks  before  it 
would  give  up,  After  an  injection  of  a  substance  that  blocks  steroid  out- 
put, the  high-steroid  strain  would  behave  like  the  low  reactors  and  would 
also  require  1 5  shocks  to  prevent  them  from  drinking  on. 

A  very  interesting  adrenal  adaptation  was  discovered  in  a  variety  of 
desert  mouse  that  showed  none  of  the  usual  elevation  in  stress  steroids 
after  shock  and  other  tests.  Here,  as  it  turned  out,  the  assays  that  were 
suitable  for  most  of  the  laboratory  mice  were  totally  inappropriate.  It  was 
not  that  this  mouse  lacked  a  stress  response,  but  that  his  glandular  func- 
tions were  differently  organized.  Steroids  usually  indicative  of  stress  were, 
in  this  mouse,  functioning  to  maintain  water  in  tissue. 

577 


Some  recent  experiments  have  indicated  that  the  startle  response  of  a 
mouse  may  be  a  good  indicator  of  his  steroid  level.  Two  strains,  one  high 
steroid  and  another  low,  have  been  tested  in  a  delicate  little  box  that 
registers  the  height  and  impact  of  a  jump.  At  1 -minute  intervals  a  cap 
pistol  is  fired.  The  reactive  mice  leap  high  at  first  and  take  some  time 
befoie  they  get  used  to  the  noise  so  that  they  no  longer  jump.  The  other 
strain  consistently  shows  a  much  smaller  startle  jump  and  takes  tittle  time 
to  habituate. 

A  recent  study  of  four  strains  has  indicated  that  the  reactive  mice 
(showing  the  most  marked  steroid  response  to  shock  and  novelty)  are  the 
ones  in  whom  infant  handling  makes  the  greatest  change.  Handled  mice 
from  these  two  strains  have  indicated  the  greatest  reduction  in  stress  upon 
testing.  The  two  strains  initially  showing  least  response  to  shock  and 
novelty  in  steroid  output  have  also  least  change  after  handling. 

Current  studies  of  genetic  factors  in  the  developing  responses  of  animals 
do  suggest  a  variety  of  differences  in  initial  responsiveness  to  environment. 
Experience  makes  littie  dent  upon  the  adrenal  response  of  low-steroid 
strains.  However,  the  high-steroid  strains  appear  to  become  "less  emo- 
tional" with  experience  and  show  less  reaction  to  stress.  It  seems  likely 
that  analogous  genetic  differences  operate  within  human  beings.  Parents, 
certainly,  speak  as  if  the  "high  strung1'  infant  makes  his  presence  known 
in  early  infancy.  The  intimations  of  a  temperament  seem  to  be  conveyed 
by  the  baby  in  the  first  weeks  of  life.  Tests,  such  as  the  startle  test  used 
with  laboratory  animals,  might  indeed  give  a  very  early  estimate  of  the 
infant's  adrenal  responsiveness.  Even  so,  one  would  not  know  whether  the 
infant's  intensity  of  reaction  were  truly  genetic  or  whether  it  were  in- 
fluenced by  its  mother's  own  steroid  levels  prior  to  delivery  and  through- 
out the  nursing  period.  In  addition,  maternal  behavior  presumably  plays 
its  role  in  shaping  of  the  infant. 

Concurrent  with  a  number  of  genetic  studies,  the  laboratory  has  also 
been  investigating  the  mother's  own  prenatal  and  postpartum  adrenal  re- 
activity and  the  effects  on  the  young. 


The  Development  of  Endocrine  Responses  in  Critical  Periods 

Whatever  the  genes  that  formed  him,  a  child  does  spend  almost  a  year 
in  the  chemical  bath  of  his  mother's  womb.  In  the  first  months  afterbirth 
he  will  be  nourished  by  her  milk  and  suffused  with  her  chemicals.  Most  of 
his  experience  will  derive  from  her  ministrations.  Will  he  feel  the  conse- 
quences if  his  mother  endures  an  ordeal  while  she  is  bearing  him?  Will  he 
suffer  from  the  debilities  of  a  high-steroid  start  in  life  if  she  is  high  strung 
while  nursing  him?  Does  her  stress  chemistry  give  the  infant's  own  physi- 
ology a  push,  helping  him  to  set  the  level  of  his  endocrine  responses  to 
life? 

There  is  a  period  in  the  life  of  an  infant  rat  which  seems  to  be  strongly 
influenced  by  the  mother's  chemistry  perhaps  through  her  milk,  perhaps 
through  her  behavior.  It  was  discovered  when  rat  pups  were  being  tested 

578 


for  their  steroid  responses  to  shock  and  other  stresses.  The  experimenters 
noticed  that  the  neonate,  on  the  day  of  birth  and  for  3  days  of  life,  gave  a 
marked  response  to  stress.  But  after  the  third  day,  there  was  no  sign  of 
adrenal  stress  hormones.  The  usual  response  had  vanished.  Ether,  electric 
shock,  exposure  to  cold-all  of  the  standard  laboratory  stresses  failed  to 
elicit  a  steroid  response  in  the  infant.  They  injected  ACTH-the  hormone 
that  instructs  the  adrenals  to  release  stress  steroids-but  nothing  seemed  to 
happen.  There  was  no  detectable  release  of  steioids  from  the  adienals.  The 
animal  was  in  a  dormant  period,  and  it  lasted  somewhere  between  6  and 
12  days. 

Handling  the  infant  seemed  to  shorten  this  dormant  period.  Creatures 
handled  in  the  laboratory  would  again  show  a  steroid  response  to  shock 
after  about  6  days.  Protected  creatures  would  not  resume  their  response 
for  about  1 2  days. 

Why  did  the  animal  give  a  shock  response  right  aftei  birth  and  then 
exhibit  none  for  almost  2  weeks?  The  probable  answer  was  that  the  fiist 
responses  depended  upon  an  endowment  of  steroids  from  the  mother,  but 
thereafter  the  creature  was  in  a  state  of  transition.  His  endocrine  system 
might  be  setting  up  its  steroid  response  levels  dictated,  in  part,  by  genetic 
endowment  mediated  by  the  mother  and  by  his  expeiiences  during  the 
critical  period.  The  experimenters  postulated  that  the  infant  animal  must 
adjust  his  adrenal  hormone  release  to  the  stresses  of  the  outside  world, 
after  a  fashion-setting  a  glandular  thermostat  that  would  dictate  the  in- 
tensity of  his  responses  to  events. 

The  research  team  began  to  take  a  close  look  at  the  mother's  influence 
upon  this  ciitical  adjustment-a  glandular  calibration  that  would  seriously 
affect  the  animal's  rate  of  growth,  his  learning  ability,  his  resistance  to 
disease,  and  his  long-term  ability  to  adapt  and  survive. 

They  saw,  with  mice*  that  the  mother's  steroid  levels  appeared  to  exert 
far  more  influence  than  the  father's.  Genetically  identical  mothers  bred  to 
antipodal  fathers  bore  infants  whose  steroid  response  patterns  inevitably 
followed  those  of  the  mothers-not  the  fathers. 

If  the  mother's  own  steroid  levels  were  mediating  the  endocrine  re- 
sponses of  her  newborn,  the  experimenters  expected  that  a  shift  in  her 
own  steroid  levels  might  show  up  in  the  young.  After  delivery,  in  one 
study,  they  injected  the  mother  with  ACTH.  The  newborn  showed  a 
higher  steroid  response  than  did  the  neonates  whose  mothers  received 
only  saline  injections.  The  next  test  was  to  drastically  reduce  the  mother's 
steroid  level  with  a  chemical  that  blocks  the  release  of  ACTH.  Her  young, 
as  might  be  expected,  exhibited  decidedly  lower  steroid  levels.  She  was 
undoubtedly  still  capable  of  transmitting  steroids,  for  an  ACTH  blocker 
would  prevent  further  release  of  the  hormones  but  would  not  reduce  the 
high  level  already  circulating  in  her  system.  The  steroid  level  of  the 
mother  distinctly  influenced  that  of  her  young. 

Ordinarily  a  mother's  stress  responses  are  at  their  lowest  JUF* 
delivery.  The  steroid  levels  rise  before  labor  and  diminish  after  t 
and  during  nursing.  After  the  offspring  are  weaned,  there  is  n 
increase  in  steroid  levels.  A  similar  pattern  of  steroid  decrease  aft 


found  in  human  beings.  Perhaps  this  is  a  built-in  safety  device.  It  works  to 
insure  low-stress  responses  in  the  mother  directly  after  birth,  Since  high 
levels  in  the  infant  will  stunt  growth,  it  is  essential  that  the  newborn  not 
experience  high-steroid  concentrations  too  early.  Afterbirth  and  through- 
out the  nursing  period  natvue  has  provided  a  mechanism  for  generating  a 
particular  calm  in  the  mother.  This  appeais  to  be  the  period  in  which  she 
mediates  the  initial  steroid  levels  of  the  young,  perhaps  through  her  be- 
havior and  her  milk. 

Within  the  laboratory,  in  a  study  of  mice,  the  calmest,  low-steroid 
mothers  had  offspring  who  showed  a  sluggish  response  to  shock.  It  has  not 
yet  been  determined  whether  these  low-steroid  infants  will  learn  discrimi- 
nations faster  than  the  more  excitable  creatures.  One  thing  was  ctear-the 
infants  reflected,  in  their  steroid  respones,  the  steroid  levels  of  (heir 
mothers.  A  good  many  factors  might  be  icsponsible  for  the  base  steroid 
fevel  of  the  mother. 

In  a  study  of  rats,  the  experimenters  started  at  the  beginning-in  the 
infancy  of  the  mothers.  They  handled  one  gioup  of  females  in  infancy, 
and  protected  another  group.  Now  the  effects  of  handling  might  be  traced 
through  to  the  next  generation,  for  all  the  mothers  were  of  the  same 
genetic  strain.  Would  the  infant  experience  of  the  mother  show  up  in  her 
own  offspring?  The  study  seemed  to  answer  in  the  affirmative.  The 
handled  mothers  had  lower  resting  levels  of  steroids,  and  their  young 
showed  low-stress  responses.  The  protected,  or  nonhandled  mothers  had 
higher  resting  levels  of  steroids,  and  their  young  showed  higher  stress 
responses.  The  handling  of  the  infant  female  appeared  to  help  set  an 
endocrine  level  that  was  then  transmitted  to  the  next  generation. 

Although  a  round  of  different  studies  has  not  yet  indicated  whether  the 
mother's  steroid  influence  comes  mainly  through  her  milk  or  from  her 
behavior-it  seems  clear  that  her  steroid  levels  dictate  the  early  levels  of 
the  infant.  A  cross-fostering  study  of  rats  indicated  very  dramatically  the 
importance  of  the  mother's  role  in  mediating  the  early  steroid  levels  of  the 
young.  The  offspring  of  high-steroid  mothers  were  given  to  low-steroid 
mothers,  and  vice  versa.  Initially,  it  seems  that  the  offspring  of  high- 
steroid  mothers  show  a  very  low-steroid  response  to  stress  when  they  are 
raised  by  low-steroid  mothers. 

The  importance  of  the  mother's  steroid  level  has  been  evident  through- 
out these  studies  and  raises  many  questions  about  infant  care  and  the 
postnatal  experience  of  the  mother.  Is  the  nursing  period  a  time  in  which 
shock  to  mothers  will  raise  their  steroid  levels  and  transmit  to  the  young  a 
tendency  for  emotionality  that  would  reverberate  throughout  infancy,  re- 
tard growth,  and  even  appear  in  the  adult  responses  to  alarm?  Although 
many  questions  must  be  answered  about  the  mother's  own  experience  and 
her  steroid  state-the  other  half  of  the  question  must  be  answered  by 
determining  what  the  infant  body  is  doing  during  this  critical  time  after 
birth. 

His  mother's  steroid  levels  give  him  an  initial  presetting,  yet  during 
these  first  weeks  experience  will  shorten  the  transition  period  and  may 
alter  his  glandular  calibration  of  intensity  and  stimulus.  The  process  might 

580 


be  likened  to  the  setting  of  an  automatic  thermostat,  so  that  it  will  turn 
on  only  when  the  temperature  drops  below  a  certain  point. 

If  a  kind  of  hormonostat  is  developed  during  the  time  when  the  infant 
begins  producing  his  own  steroids,  its  effect  upon  his  behavior  may  come 
about  through  a  feedback  to  the  central  nervous  system.  When  his  steroid 
levels  rise,  his  brain  will  move  him  in  the  diiection  of  alertness,  of  excita- 
bility. This  early  development  of  the  hormonal  system,  according  to  the 
data  now  amassed,  would  seem  to  exert  a  pi o found  and  endless  effect  on 
the  growth,  the  adaptability,  and  health  of  the  animal.  Basic  metaboJic 
processes  may  be  altered  through  this  system  and  so  the  steroid  response 
to  shock  or  other  stress  may  be  the  key  to  many  of  the  adult  animal's 
adaptive  capacities. 

The  hormones  issued  by  the  adrenals,  in  response  to  pituitary  command 
during  situations  of  potential  danger,  aie  evidently  affected  by  the  sex 
hormones.  Normal  male  rodents  when  shocked  show  a  lower  stress  re- 
sponse than  do  the  noimal  females.  The  typical  pattern  of  reaction  diffeis 
according  to  sex.  The  male  reacts  by  pouring  steroids  into  his  system 
much  more  lapidly  and  more  rapidly  declines  when  the  stimulus  is  over. 
The  female  takes  longer  to  respond  and  characteristically  shows  high- 
steroid  levels  for  some  time  after  the  shock.  This  description  of  stress 
reaction  may  sound  familiar  for  it  describes  a  male-female  difference  in 
reaction  that  is  encountered  in  the  human  species.  The  typical  emotional 
female  remains  emotional  long  after  her  baby  has  returned  home  safely  or 
the  near  accident  has  been  averted.  One  might  almost  expect  that  the 
difference  between  the  male  and  female  stress  pattern  is  a  pivot  of  conflict 
between  the  sexes.  Surely  it  is  clear  that  the  hormones  of  stress  are  in 
some  way  related  to  the  apportionment  of  sex  hormones, 

Just  as  the  stress  responses  of  an  organism  appear  to  be  shaped  during 
critical  periods  of  infancy  and  remain  irreversible  thereafter,  the  shaping 
of  sexual  traits  by  hormonal  settings  is  irreversible  after  a  critical  period  in 
infancy. 


Sex  Hormones  and  Behavior 

The  sex  of  a  child  can  be  determined  very  early-while  still  in  the 
womb.  The  overt  sexual  features  are  pronounced  by  birth,  and  a  doctor 
can  after  one  glance  say,  "It's  a  boy."  Nevertheless,  the  maleness  of  the 
child  may  be  deceptive.  His  internal  hormonal  states  will  determine 
whether  he  acts  like  a  boy  or  a  girl.  Quite  a  number  of  experiments  have 
demonstrated  that  sex  can  be  altered  aftei  birth. 

Sexual  differentiation  determines  the  behavior  and  reproductive  ability 
of  the  adult,  also  the  nature  of  physical  responses  to  stress,  «nH  thk 


female  animals  grew  up  without  any  sign  of  the  usual  cstrus  cycle,  hut 
rather  seemed  to  be  in  a  continuous  estrus.  They  did  not,  however,  show 
cyclical  iclease  of  important  hormones  and  ncvei  became  prugmml  al- 
though given  ample  opportunity,  and  so  it  was  thought  that  these  jinimata 
refused  to  mate.  As  latei  studies  were  to  demonstrate,  the  transplanting  uf 
testes  was  unnecessary.  A  single  injection  of  testosteione   in    a  fcwiikj 
during  her  first  5  days  of  life  sufficed  to  create  a  lack  of  cstrus  cycle  niu) 
an  inability  to  be  impregnated.  Hormone  ticatmcnts  to  the  mother  in 
other  studies  indicated  that  prenatal  estiogcn  could  produce  fcniini/uljoii 
in  the  male  offspring,  and  testosteione  resulted  in  mate  behaviors  in  IV- 
males.  By  the  mid-1  950's,  it  seemed  clear  that  in  the  life  of  the  guinwi  pig 
there  was  a  critical  period  during  its  first  week  when  sex  hormones  could 
enter  the  brain  and  influence  the  sexual  differentiation  of  the  nniniitl. 

Dr.  Levine's  laboratory  has  been  using  several  techniques  to  find  out 
how,  indeed,  the  hormones  cause  sexual  differentiation  «nd  (hereby  in- 
fluence behavior.  The  investigator  had  found  that  when  mule  rats  were 
given  the  ovarian  hormone,  estrogen,  their  gonads  atrophied,  und  (hey 
were  forever  feminized.  Indeed,  if  the  male  rat  was  castrated  during  his 
first  24  hours,  it  was  possible  to  implant  an  ovary  in  him  in  adulthood  nnd 
the  ovary  would  ovulate  in  the  cyclical  fashion  typical  of  female  cstrus. 

Further  transplantation  studies  suggested  that  hormones  must  inl'Iut-neo 
the  brain.  The  cyclic  pattern  of  ovulation  is  stimulated  by  the  piluilnry 
gland  in  the  base  of  the  brain.  This  gland  produces  the  foWdc-sliimilntinu 
hormone  that  prepares  the  ovary  to  produce  egg  cells.  When  (his  process  is 
finished,  the  pituitary  issues  a  luteinizing  hormone  which  ripens  the  folli- 
cles so  that  they  rupture  and  release  the  eggs.  When  a  female  ovary  is 
transplanted  mto  a  male  who  has  been  castrated  nconatally,  that  oviiry 
will  ovulate  This  suggests  that  neither  the  ovary  nor  the  pituitar  alone  Is 


gess     a   neer  te  ovary  nor  the  pituitary  alone  Is 
°r  *'1*     °l 


acuo    on  . 

react  to  hormonal  stimulation  and  that  during  critical  periods  (how  brain 

" 


wtiMn  .  final  «'no,i 

rats  v^ffi^S^^f"0*';1'8110™01108'  Whon  rclllillu 
sterone,  in  infancy  hev^n,!  ?'  mal°  Eomidal  hormone,  leslo- 
normal  sex  b^SrsUtevL  °°  "^"^  ""<'  ™^^  »b~ 

they  would  growTnto  id"  K  ItT"  *?"  W?  d°SCS  ol'  l«Mlostoro»c, 
Any  observefwatSg  on  of  tt!  e  ,'10  tnco,?ta™*  ^nlo  boj.uvtors 
interacted  with  another  feLte  ,  th  horraomlllr  tr™^  romalos  ».  she 
*e  was  .  male.  CmoS  h  ,*  v  C<1ge  *?'1llld  llilV°  to  COIldl1^-  """ 
pearance  of  ejaculations  o  of  femTh^°  "  ^  C°"cludo  '"  tllc  1^ 
would  not  reverse  the  effw?  ™?frm  J  ?orn)on°-Mt«>8cn-[n  odulthoocl 
ever  after  would  behave  as  a  mak  '  8'W"  lestostcro'^  '"  infancy, 


582 


These  studies  suggested  thai  hormonal  balance  in  infancy  was  crucial, 
and  that  its  effects  upon  the  central  nervous  system  would  subsequently 
influence  behavior  and  reproduction.  The  castrated  neonate— effectively 
losing  his  testosterone—appeared  to  be  suffering  from  an  alteration  in  the 
brain  tissues  which  would  Hilcr  become  receptive  to  male  hormones.  This 
male,  it  should  be  said,  was  being  feminized  in  many  ways.  He  would 
subsequently  show  feminine  responses  in  nonsexual  behaviois.  His 
hormonal  balance,  causing  differentiation  within  bruin  tissue  would  also 
affect  his  adrenal  system. 

Male  and  female  rats  differ  in  their  open-field  behavior,  but  the  dif- 
ference can  be  reversed  with  a  single  hormone  shot.  A  castiatcd  neonate 
will  act  like  a  female  in  the  open  field.  A  female  injected  with  testosterone 
will  act  like  a  male,  The  activity  cycles  of  the  male  and  female  rat  ordi- 
narily show  characteristic  differences,  for  the  female  becomes  quite  active 
before  cstrus  and  inactive  afterwards.  A  castrated  neonate  with  trans- 
planted ovary  will  show  female  activity  cycles. 

A  variety  of  studies  has  indicated  thai  these  changes  in  sexual  orienta- 
tion occur  during  a  very  short  time  after  birth  in  the  rat.  Moieovcr,  the 
critical  period  is  especially  brief  for  the  male.  Male  differentiation  seems 
to  occur  during  the  first  48  hours,  while  female  differentiation  takes  as 
long  as  120  hours  in  the  same  species.  The  investigators  have  postulated 
that  the  female  state  is  in  some  sense  more  primitive  and  that  without  the 
addition  of  a  particular  male  hormone  every  rat  remains  a  female. 

The  evidence  comes  from  a  number  of  studies.  In  essence,  it  seems  to  be 
lack  of  testosterone  that  creates  female  differentiation,  A  newborn  male 
rat,  if  castrated,  will  not  produce  testosterone.  If  he  is  given  no  supple- 
menting injections  of  testosterone  he  will  behave  as  a  female,  exhibit 
female  sex  behavior  with  the  lordosis  position  of  receptivity;  he  will  ac- 
cept implanted  ovaries,  show  ovulation,  and  behave  like  a  female  on  open- 
field  tests,  in  response  to  shock,  and  with  other  females.  Injections  of  the 
female  hormone,  estrogen,  do  noi  have  the  overwhelming  impact  on  a 
male  that  is  caused  by  Inck  of  testosterone.  Hslrogcn  causes  the  testes  to 
wither,  however,  and  creates  some  female  behavior. 

Hormonal  influences  can  be  profound  after  birth,  for  it  appears  that 
sexual  functions  and  behaviors  arc  exceedingly  malleable  during  critical 
periods.  Rats  in  prenatal  development,  arc  structurally  developed  in  part 
by  the  influence  of  androgcn.  Written  into  the  very  chromosomes  are  the 
directions  that  determine  whether  the  animal  will  have  testes  or  ovaries. 
Once  the  testes  and  ovaries  develop,  however,  their  hormones  exert  the 
final  influence  upon  the  sexual  nature  of  the  animal.  During  infancy,  the 
hormones  perform  an  organizational  function,  perhaps  by  creating  a  regu- 
latory system  within  the  brain  that  finally  governs  sex  behavior  and  biolo- 
gical function.  Gonadal  hormones  appear  to  act  upon  the  brain,  thus 
dictating  whether  the  creature  shall  be  male  or  female. 

By  this  differentiation,  the  sex  hormones  dictate  many  behaviors  that 
are  not  strictly  sexual.  Recent  studies  within  this  laboratory  have  indi- 
cated how  estrogen  and  testosterone  injections  will  influence  the  emo- 
tional behavior  of  the  animal.  Inbred  strains  of  highly  reactive  rats  and 

583 


unreactive  rats,  as  defined  by  open-field  behavior,  were  injected  with  hor- 
mones and  observed  in  their  subsequent  open-field  behavior.  Before  injec- 
tion, the  reactive  males  defecated  more  than  the  females.  In  all  the  strains 
tested,  the  females  weie  more  active.  Neonatal  hormone  injection  had  a 
profound  effect  on  the  nonsexual  behavior  of  the  animals.  Estrogen 
caused  considerably  more  defecation  and  activity,  producing  what  might 
be  called  a  more  emotional  rat,  and  affected  both  sexes  by  heightening 
this  reactivity.  Testosterone,  on  the  other  hand,  produced  a  more  male 
response  by  one  criteria;  it  increased  defecation  in  the  reactive  females  to 
roughly  the  level  of  the  males,  In  another  study,  male  neonates  had  been 
injected  with  estrogen,  and  when  stressed,  their  output  of  corticosterone 
was  like  that  of  the  female. 

In  current  studies,  the  investigatois  have  been  exploring  for  reliable 
behavior  differences  among  male  and  female  rats.  It  will  then  be  possible 
to  perform  more  sensitive  tests  of  the  effects  of  hormone  changes,  In 
mice,  aggressive  behavior  is  exhibited  mainly  by  males.  Males  fight  when 
placed  together  but  females  do  not.  Females  given  testosterone,  however, 
will  engage  in  some  fighting,  Further  studies  may  indicate  whether  neo- 
natal castration  will  remove  fighting  from  the  behavior  repertoire  of  the 
male  and  whether  neonatal  injection  of  testosterone  produces  an  adult 
female  who  does  battle  like  a  male.  Female  rodents  show  more  rapid 
avoidance  conditioning  than  males  and  this,  too,  may  turn  out  to  be 
amenable  to  hormonal  change. 

Stress  responses  have  been  alteicd  by  hormonal  treatment.  In  measuring 
the  adrenal  steroids  in  the  blood,  samples  are  drawn  from  the  jugular  vein 
under  ether.  This  procedure,  in  itself,  constitutes  a  stress  for  the  animal. 
Ordinarily,  the  normal  male  will  respond  to  the  ether  and  surgical  cut  by  a 
rise  in  corticosterone  lasting  about  20-45  minutes.  The  male  treated  with 
neonatal  estrogen  shows  almost  double  the  normal  concentration  of  adre- 
nal stress  hormone,  but  his  response  is  still  considerably  less  than  that 
generated  by  the  normal  female.  If,  however,  the  female  has  been  given 
testosterone  during  the  initial  days  of  life,  her  output  of  adrenal  stress 
steroid  will  fall  between  that  of  the  normal  male  and  the  estrogen-treated 
male.  It  might  seem  that  testosterone,  acting  upon  the  central  nervous 
system,  mediates  the  response  of  the  emergency  system,  the  emotional, 
or  stress  output  of  the  animal.  There  are  many  questions  and  one  very 
striking  conclusion  to  be  drawn  from  this  program  of  research.  The  en- 
vironment of  the  newborn  animal  and  its  changes  have  a  very  significant 
and  life-long  effect  upon  his  adult  biology  and  behavior.  Sex  hormones, 
the  interplay  of  environment  factors,  and  adrenal  steroids,  all  appear  to 
have  an  organizing  influence  upon  the  central  nervous  system  of  the  new- 
born. Hormones  cause  the  infant  to  become  a  male  or  female  in  function, 
And  infantile  stimulation  appears  to  organize  the  emotional  responses  of 
the  creature. 

Both  neonatal  manipulation  and  hormonal  treatment  may  have  an 
effect  upon  the  neuroendocrine  system  as  a  whole,  for  they  are  inter- 
linked, An  event  occurs,  and  a  brain  activity  signals  the  pituitary  to  release 
ACTH,  which  in  turn  stimulates  the  adrenal  cortex  to  produce  steroids. 

584 


The  pituitary  produces  growth  hormone,  and  hormone  enabling  the 
ovary  to  produce  eggs,  and  a  male  hoi  mone  that  stimulates  the  production 
of  testosterone  in  the  cells  of  the  lesles.  I  he  hormones  of  the  pituitary 
stimulate  the  thyroid  gland.  Thus  the  systems  ol  endocrine  regulation  and 
hormonal  regulation  of  mating  behavior  and  specific  sex  mil  functions  me 
to  some  extent  joined.  The  n comic,  at  critical  periods,  win  be  shifted  in  his 
responses  to  stress  by  manipulation,  treatment  wilh  lulrcnal  steroids,  01  by 
gonadal  hormones.  These  iKHirophysiolo^ical  systems,  wilh  (heir  enor- 
mous impact  upon  adult  behavior,  lie  ul  UK-  huh  of  the  formative  periods. 

The  mysterious  and  vast  influence  ol  (he  honnones  has  been  probed  at 
the  cellular  level  by  many  biologists.  Today  I  here  is  11  growing  body  of 
data  that  suggests  how  Ihc  hormones  exeil  their  influence.  Hormones 
within  the  tissues  appear  Lo  direct  the  activity  of  tin-  t-cnes,  the  controllers 
Df  individual  cells.  It  is  by  their  influence  upon  Hie  gene  that  the 
lormoncs  appear  to  steer  cell  differentiation,  Thus  (he  hormone,  while 
jxternal  to  the  cell,  can  niter  (he  |>n  Item  of  uene  activity  am!  (hereby 
nfluence  protein  synthesis.  This  influence  is  nowheie  more  clearly  seen 
:han  in  some  of  the  recent  t  mnspliintalioii  experiments  in  which  a  eas- 
:rated  male  wears  a  healthy,  cyclically  nourished  ovary  in  an  eye-socket, 
vhile  a  testosterone-treated  female  remains  amwulalory  and  shows  no 
cycles. 

These  studies  demonstrate  llu-  power  of  m-omilnl  hormones  in  shaping 
he  sexual  differentiation  of  the  animal.  Insofar  as  Hie  mother's  own 
jalancc  may  influence  the  infant,  there  aie  some  very  strong  implications 
or  medicine.  It  would  seem  thai  an  indiscriminate  use  of  prenatal 
iormon.cs  holds  some  possible  dangers  for  Hie  differentiation  of  male 
nfants,  since  the  male  appears  particularly  vulnerable  to  lack  of  Icsloslcr- 
>nc  or  to  an  overbalance  of  female  hormones.  Uecauso  neonatal  honnones 
ppear  to  play  such  a  cleeisive  role  in  sexual  dilTci'eiitiitlion,  hormone 
Indies  of  infancy  should  instigate  new  approaches  and  reseaieh  into  sex- 
lal  deviation  and  its  possible  (real  men  Is.  If  it  is  possible  to  develop  sensi- 
ive  hormone  assays  for  the  infant  aiul  child,  it  seems  possible  that  prcvcn- 
ive  treatment  might  be  attempted.  Surely,  these  studies  raise  important 
tueslions  about  the  extent  to  which  deviant  behaviois  are  the  result  of 
lormonal  imbalances  that  have  gone  undetected  in  the  clinic  but  have 
:d  to  a  life  of  social  cnstigulion  niul  punishment.  To  what  extent  might 
fiesc  hormonal  imbalances  be  caused  by  the  sleroiils  ingested  by  pregnant 
mothers'?  To  what  extent  might  they  he  altered  after  the  critical  and 
orirmtivc  periods  have  passed.  Sexual  differentiation  is  crucial  to  human 
djustmcnl.  It  may  seem  ama/ing  that  the  Mory  of  neonatal  hormones  and 
licir  bearing  on  differentiation  and  behavior  have  just  begun  to  be  ex- 
•lorcd  in  the  animal  laboratory,  but  the  liummi  being  for  whom  those 
icchanisms  have  long  social  and  personal  reverberations  is  fur  loss  under- 
Loocl  und  studied. 

The  psychophysiology  of  the  luiiunn  formative  period  will  undoubtedly 
:ikc  its  guidelines  from  the  animal  laboratory.  Already  studies  performed 
y  this  laboratory  suggest  techniques  thai  should  enable  safe  and  Illumina- 
ng  studies  of  human  infants.  It  should  be  possible  to  devise  an  equivalent 

585 


of  the  startle  apparatus  permitting  measurements  of  the  amplitude  of  an 
infant's  response  and  the  detection  of  early  signs  of  overreactivity.  By 
techniques  allowing  a  determination  of  steroid  concentrations  within  a 
diop  of  blood,  it  should  be  possible  to  measure  the  steroid  elevations  an 
infant  shows  in  response  to  mild  stimulation.  Thus  we  may  hope  to  dis- 
cover something  about  the  critical  periods  in  which  the  neuroendocrine 
system  sets  up  its  hierarchy.  Controlled  studies  of  infants  and  mothers 
should  give  us  information  about  the  prenatal  and  postnatal  influences 
and  how  they  are  molded  by  outside  events.  It  should  be  possible  to  detect 
the  eavly  warning  signals  of  hypeneactivity  in  a  child  pretending  sus- 
ceptibility to  sickness  and  perhaps  to  psychosomatic  illness.  We  can  finally 
hope  to  establish  child-watching  and  child-reaving  practices  that  will  en- 
hance the  adaptability  and  health  of  the  individual. 

Some  of  the  more  mysteiious  questions  of  large  populations  may 
finally  be  answered  as  more  data  become  available  from  studies  of  the 
ways  in  which  hormone  mechanisms  influence  differentiation.  Indeed,  it 
may  be  possible  to  answei  some  of  the  recurrent  questions  of  the  demo- 
grapher. During  war  and  its  aftermath,  for  instance,  it  has  been  noticed 
that  theie  is  an  increase  in  male  births.  Is  this  a  response  of  the  parents' 
hormonal  systems  to  stress? 

These  studies  of  the  neuroendocrine  system  in  its  developmental  stages 
have  far-i caching  implications,  but  perhaps  the  ones  that  fall  closest  to 
home  are  really  hopeful  questions-what  do  high  doses  of  steroids  do  to 
the  piegnant  mother  and  her  young?  How  could  our  clinics  begin  to 
obtain  predictive  information  about  the  endocrine  adjustments  of  the 
newborn?  Can  we  begin  to  look  for  hormone  balance  and  stress-steroid 
concentrations  as  usefully  as  we  look  for  blood  types? 

Research  Grants.   Mil  1051,  MH  1630,  K3-MII  19,936,  MH  7435 
Dates  of  Interviews:  Fall,  1965;spring,  1966 

References: 

Davidson,  E.  H.  Hormones  and  genes.  Scient.  American,  1965,  212,  36-45. 

Hanis,  G.  W.>  &  Levine,  S.  Sexual  differentiation  of  the  biain  and  its  experimental 

control,/  PhysioL  1962,  163,  42-43. 
Click,  D,,  Von  Redlich,  Dorothy,  &  Levine,  S.  Fluorometric  determination  of  corti- 

costerone  and  cortisol  In  Q.Q2-O.Q5  milHHters  of  plasma  01  submilligram  samples  of 

adrenal  tissue.  Endocrinology,  1964,  74,  653-655. 
Gray,  J.  A,,  &  Levine,  S.  Effect  of  induced  oestrus  on  emotional  behavior  in  selected 

strains  of  rats.  Nature,  1964,  201,  1198-1200. 
Gray,  J.  A.,  Levine,  S.,  &  Broadhurst,  P.  L.  Gonadal  hormone  injections  in  infancy  and 

adult  emotional  behavior.  A  mm,  Behav,,  1965,  13,  1,  33-45. 
Levine,  S.  Stimulation  in  infancy.  Scient.  American,  1 960,  202,  80-86. 
Levine,  S.  The  effects  of  infantile  experience  on  adult  behavior.  In  A.  J.  Bachrach 

(Ed,),  Experimental  foundations  of  clinical  psychology.  New  York:  Basic  Books, 

1962. 
Levine,  S.  The  psyche-physiological  effects  of  infantile  stimulation.  In  E.  Bliss  (Ed.), 

Roots  of  behavio).  New  York:  Hocbei,  1962. 
Levine,  S.  Some  effects  of  stimulation  in  infancy.  In  S,  H,  Bartlett  (Ed,),  Lessons  from 

Animal  Behavior  foi  the  Clinician.  London:  Nat'l  Spastics  Soc,  (Little  Club  Clinics 

in  Developmental  Medicine,  No.  7),  1962,  18-24. 

586 


I.evinc,  S.,  &  HnmillnnM,  I1.  I  .  (it-ru'lk  uml  oiiloneiuMic  determinants  of  adult  be- 
havior In  llic  ml  ,f  <'<>int>.  riivxt'il.  /'.NT/HI/.,  |»)d.l,  Sf>,  <123-4iR. 

I.cvim1,  S.,  A  Miillius,  K.  l-sirtt|ton  iitlniinisti'rcil  nvnniilally  aflccts  ;uliill  sexual  be- 
havior in  niiik  iiiul  li'inali'  nils,  >Vnr;iM',  I''M,  I'l-l,  IKS-IK7. 

Ijjvinw,  S..  A  t'lt'lnuui.  I),  M  OiltViontlul  ptiiMiiu  vorlicnsU'ronc  response  to  stress  in 
lour  inliR-il  iti.nnMil  niici1.  /'.'m/iit'/imi/fw,  I''M,7,S,  U2-M4. 

.ovine,  S.  A  Wot/rl.  A.  Itituntili-  i-xpt'iii'iHVi,  stniin  tlitlt'ivncus,  and  avoklnncc  learn- 
/'/M'u.i/ /'.u'c/H'/,,  I'Kil,  Sti.  «7»>HK1. 


587 


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 

Major  contributions  in  the  editing  and  preparation  of  this  volume  weie 
made  by  Sherry  Prestwich  and  Muriel  Reich.  They  were  assisted  by  Emily 
Barren,  Lillian  Becker,  Mary  Carmody,  Helen  Fussell,  and  Sandy  Snider, 


588 

ft  U.S.  GOVERNMENT  CHINTING  OFFICE  .  l»7f  O—Hll-tltl