Skip to main content

Full text of "Notes on the development of a child .."

See other formats


fi 
life 


:|iHWHiii; 


§ 


lllf  Il|p: 
IpIi  llpjplj- 


iffi 


Hi 


ilill 


Iflll 

III 

fill 


MHiJni!!? 


llflilf'- 

Iflfffi  I 

lli 

«  III 

■      i 


:fl)emorj><rf 
Eleanor 
Scheson 

fly&tllocH 
©amble 


(Bitf 

of 
Slcramira 
(gamble 
iDuke 


Notes  on 
The   Development  of  a  Child 

By 
Milicent  Washburn  Shinn,  Cand.  Phil. 


)W«T 


!5"  1 


SCIlNCE 

U 
1131 

ss 


1 


INTRODUCTION 


It  is  a  well-recognized  fact  in  the  history  of  science  that  the 
very  subjects  which  concern  our  dearest  interests,  which  lie  near- 
est our  hearts,  are  exactly  those  which  are  the  last  to  submit  to 
scientific  methods,  to  be  reduced  to  scientific  law.  Thus  it  has 
come  to  pass  that  while  babies  are  born  and  grow  up  in  every 
household,  and  while  the  gradual  unfolding  of  their  faculties  has 
been  watched  with  the  keenest  interest  and  intensest  joy  by  in- 
telligent and  even  scientific  fathers  and  mothers  from  time  imme- 
morial, yet  very  little  has  yet  been  done  in  the  scientific  study 
of  this  most  important  of  all"  possible  subjects,  —  the  ontogenic 
evolution  of  the  faculties  of  the  human  mind.  Only  in  the  last 
few  years  has  scientific  attention  been  drawn  to  the  subject  at 
all.  Its  transcendent  importance  has  already  enlisted  many  ob- 
servers, but  on  account  of  the  great  complexity  of  the  phenom- 
ena, and  still  more  the  intrinsic  difficulty  of  their  interpretation, 
scientific  progress  has  scarcely  yet  commenced. 

What  is  wanted  most  of  all  in  this,  as  in  every  science,  is  a 
body  of  carefully  observed  facts.  But  to  be  an  accomplished  in- 
vestigator in  this  field  requires  a  rare  combination  of  qualities. 
There  must  be  a  wide  intelligence,  combined  with  patience  in 
observing  and  honesty  in  recording.  There  must  be  also  an 
earnest  scientific  spirit,  a  loving  sympathy  with  the  subject  of  in- 
vestigation, yet  under  watchful  restraint,  lest  it  cloud  the  judg- 
ment;  keenness  of  intuitive  perception,  yet  soberness  of  judgment 
in  interpretation. 

Now  I  am  quite  convinced,  from  my  intimate  acquaintance 
with  her,  and  especially  from  a  careful  examination  of  her  work, 

(iii) 


iv  Introduction. 

that  Miss  Shinn  possesses  many  of  these  qualities  in  an  eminent 
degree.  The  careful,  painstaking,  patient,  intelligent  character  of 
her  observations  must  be  evident  to  every  reader.  The  perfect 
honesty  of  the  record  and  the  really  earnest  scientific  spirit  in 
which  the  investigation  is  undertaken  and  pursued  to  the  end, 
are  equally  certain.  I  am  sure,  too,  I  easily  detect  evidence  of 
the  loving,  sympathetic  relation  with  the  subject,  necessary  for 
insight  and  yet  not  sufficient  to  obscure  the  judgment. 

I  am  quite  convinced,  therefore,  that  the  observations  herein 
recorded  are  thoroughly  reliable.  If  so,  it  is  impossible  to  over- 
estimate their  importance.  Of  course,  interpretation  must  go 
hand  in  hand  with  observation  and  record ;  and  interpretation  in 
a  subject  so  difficult,  must  be  more  or  less  doubtful ;  but  Miss 
Shinn  has  shown  singular  wisdom  in  the  caution  and  modesty 
with  which  she  draws  her  conclusions.  I  feel  quite  confident 
that  the  work  as  a  whole  deserves,  and  will  receive,  the  thought- 
ful attention  of  Psychologists  as  a  valuable  addition  to  the  ma- 
terials of  their  science. 

Joseph  Le  Conte. 


NOTES  ON   THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF 
A  CHILD. 

The  child  of  whose  development  the  following  record  was  kept 
was  born  and  has  continuously  lived  on  a  fruit  ranch  near  Niles,  in 
the  neighborhood  of  San  Francisco  Bay.  Her  parentage  is  purely 
American,  all  four  grandparents  being  descended  from  early  colo- 
nists, —  three  from  New  England  settlers,  the  fourth  from  New  Jer- 
sey and  Virginia  Quakers.  I  have  a  detailed  record  of  the  condi- 
tions of  health  and  longevity  of  her  kin  for  two  generations  pre- 
ceding her;  in  general  these  were  good,  and  there  is  in  none  of  the 
four  grandparental  lines  any  family  tendency  to  disease  of  any  sort. 

The  temperaments  of  both  parents  are  sanguine,  fond  of  pleasure 
and  change.  The  education  of  the  father  was  a  university  special 
course  in  history  and  sociology,  taken  after  thirty  years  of  age;  the 
earlier  education  was  considerable  but  unsystematic,  mainly  literary 
and  historical.  That  of  the  mother  was  systematic  through  the 
high  school  period,  ending  at  seventeen  years.  The  occupation  of 
the  father  up  to  the  time  of  her  birth  had  been  chiefly  journalistic 
and  literary;  of  the  mother,  teaching  for  two  years  before  marriage; 
her  remoter  ancestors  were  almost  all  farming  and  seafaring  people. 
The  ages  of  her  father  and  mother  at  the  time  of  her  birth  were  re- 
spectively thirty -eight  and  twenty-two  years. 

She  was  at  birth  a  strong,  active,  good-natured  baby,  without 
defect,  and  her  health  has  been  (now  up  to  the  last  quarter  of  the 
third  year)  practically  perfect.  She  was  born  two  weeks  late,  —  a 
point  that  may  have  some  bearing  on  the  rapidity  of  early  develop- 
ment. The  conditions  of  climate  and  opportunities  for  outdoor  life 
have  been  singularly  favorable. 

She  has  been  the  only  child  in  a  large  household  of  grown  peo- 
ple, and  the  object  of  a  great  deal  of  attention.  She  has  never  been 
for  an  hour  in  the  care  of  a  servant,  has  never  been  secluded  in  a 
nursery,  but  kept  in  the  midst  of  the  family.     She  has  been  taken 

(5) 


6  University  of  California.  [Vol.  .. 

about  a  great  deal  with  her  parents  upon  short  railroad  and  hotel 
trips.  The  general  tendency  of  her  environment  has  been  in  my 
judgment  toward  developing  a  liking  for  change  and  excitement, 
and  unfavorable  to  continuous  attention. 

The  record  as  here  given  is  an  abstract  of  copious  notes  kept 
from  day  to  day,  set  down  in  many  cases  instantly  upon  the  occur- 
rence, and  always  very  promptly.  I  have  admitted  in  a  few  instances 
occurrences  reported  to  me  by  the  child's  mother  and  grandmother, 
but  none  reported  by  any  other  person  (except  two  in  the  first 
month  by  the  nurse);  in  these  cases  I  have  always  stated  that  the 
note  was  at  second  hand.  I  am  under  obligation  throughout  to 
the  mother  for  invaluable  co-operation. 


Shinn.J 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


MEASUREMENTS.1 


Weight. 


Height. 


Height. 


At 


birth 
i  mo 

2 

3 
4 
5 
6 

7 
8 

9 

io 

I  [ 

12 


9  lbs.  (naked)  ... 
II     "    (in  clothes) 

io     "        

15     "        

(measurement  unsatis 
i7-'+  lbs 

'9'+  "  

20  "  

21  "  

21^  "  

21  "      

22  "      

23  "     


19    in. 

I9K  " 
21^  " 

23  " 
factory) 

24  in. 

25  " 

26  yz  " 

27?i  " 

27  " 

27'A  " 

28  " 

2S>^    " 


At  131 

"  14 
"  15 
"  16 

"  17 
"   iS 

"  19 
"  20 
"  21 
"  22 
"  23 
"  24 


22 }4  lbs 

23 

24K    " 

25        " 

25X    " 

26 

26 

26 

27        " 

27/2     " 
28 


28X  in 
29 
29K 
29  y4 

30 
31 
31 
3i 

3  2 '4 

3-;( 
i3 


Girth  around  breast,  in  first  month,  15  inches;  at  six  months 
\J)4  inches;  at  one  year,  18^  inches. 

The  measurements  of  height  during  the  first  year  seemed  to 
me  of  little  value,  because  the  child's  struggling  disconcerted  them, 
in  spite  of  every  care.  Each  measurement,  however,  was  repeated 
three  or  four  times.  They  were  taken  by  holding  the  baby  straight 
upon  a  sheet  of  cardboard,  the  head  against  a  fixed  point,  and 
marking  at  the  heels.  The  measurement  of  height  at  eight  months 
seems  certainly  wrong. 

At  twelve  months  the  measurement  was  taken  both  by  laying 
her  upon  a  sheet  of  cardboard,  and  by  having  her  stand  against  the 
wall.  Standing  she  measured  28  inches.  During  the  second  year, 
the  measurements  were  taken  standing,  each  one  three  times  or 
more.  The  child  was  interested  in  the  process,  and  tried  to  stand 
still  and  straight. 

I  have  compared  the  record  of  weight  carefully  with  my  notes 


Following  Galton's  charts. 


8  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

as  to  the  child's  health,  appetite,  spirits,  etc.,  but  without  finding  a 
constant  relation.  During  the  first  half  year  the  weight  increases 
without  any  marked  fluctuation,  though  between  the  third  and  fifth 
months  the  ratio  declines,  and  this  may  be  due  to  the  fact  that  near 
the  end  of  the  fifth  month  the  mother's  nursing  was  slightly  supple- 
mented by  other  food ;  after  this  change  she  was  a  little  fretful  for 
about  a  week;  she  also  had  two  teeth  coming,  which  were  cut  just 
after  the  close  of  the  month.  In  the  ninth  and  tenth  months  six 
more  teeth  were  cut,  and  during  these  months  and  the  eleventh  she 
was  weaned;  during  the  eleventh  her  appetite  was  noticeably  dimin- 
ished, and  she  had  several  slight  colds  and  touches  of  digestive 
derangement,  and  during  the  twelfth  a  persistent  cold.  Yet  the 
only  month  that  shows  any  falling  off  in  weight,  or  even  any  marked 
check  in  increase,  is  the  tenth. 

In  the  second  year  again  the  only  month  that  shows  a  decrease 
of  weight  is  the  thirteenth,  in  which  no  reason  appeared  for  this  be- 
yond a  slight  digestive  derangement  in  the  third  week,  with  im- 
paired appetite.  During  the  fifteenth  month  the  increase  in  weight 
is  especially  large  and  an  increase  of  appetite  was  noticed;  yet  by 
this  month  dentition  had  fairly  begun  again,  and  the  first  molar 
came  through  early  in  the  month.  In  the  rest  of  the  year,  the  in- 
crease in  weight  month  by  month  is  very  uniform1  except  for  a 
check  in  the  nineteenth  and  twentieth,  and  another  somewhere 
between  the  twenty-first  and  twenty-third.  Yet  the  teething  was 
distributed  quite  equally  through  these  months  to  the  end  of  the 
nineteenth,  with  accompanying  rash;  and  she  had  more  or  less  cold 
each  month  before  the  twentieth.  The  severest  cold  and  cough 
was  in  the  nineteenth  month,  and  was  quite  enough  to  account 
for  the  check  in  increase  of  weight;  but  no  corresponding  reason 
for  the  continued  check  in  the  twentieth  appeared;  nor,  on  the 
other  hand,  does  any  marked  increase  in  weight  attend  the  perfect 
physical  vigor  after  the  completion  of  dentition.  In  the  seven- 
teenth month  and  thereafter  till  the  twenty-third,  a  vague  loss  in 
gayety  and  physical  buoyancy  was  quite  perceptible,  though  there 
was  almost  no  fretfulness,  and  the  child's  muscular  strength  was 
considerable,  for   late    in  the    eighteenth    month   (538th  day)  she 

'More  rigidly  so  as  the  scales  recorded  no  fractions  under  half  pounds. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  g 

picked  up  with  one  hand  a  flatiron,  which  weighed  seven  pounds, 
and  walked  off  a  yard  or  two  with  it.  In  the  latter  part  of  the 
year,  and  especially  the  twenty-fourth  month,  she  seemed  over- 
flowing with  physical  energy,  often  jumping  about  and  squealing 
in  sheer  exuberance  of  spirits.  Yet  her  increase  of  weight  was  the 
same  in  the  seventeenth  and  twenty-fourth  months. 

A  weekly  record  would  very  likely  give  clearer  results. 


SIGHT. 

i.     Sensibility  to  Light. 

The  child  seemed  quite  conscious  of  the  difference  between  light 
and  darkness  the  first  day.  At  about  an  hour  old,  she  stopped  cry- 
ing instantly  when  a  cover  was  lifted  from  her  face;  and  her  eyes 
certainly  turned  toward  any  person  who  came  near  her,  from  this 
time  on  through  the  first  and  second  weeks.  As  she  did  not  really 
look  at  anyone  before  the  fourth  week,  I  could  only  suppose  that 
she  saw  an  approaching  person  as  an  interruption  of  the  light;  her 
eyes  turned  toward  us,  however,  when  we  did  not  pass  between  her 
and  the  window.  The  nurse  said  that  her  head  and  eyes  turned 
toward  the  lamp  from  the  first  night  on. 

In  the  third,  fourth,  and  fifth  weeks  she  gazed  at  light  surfaces 
with  apparent  satisfaction,  especially  the  light  of  the  lamp  on  the 
ceiling,  the  face  of  one  holding  her,  if  it  was  turned  toward  the 
light,  and  most  of  all  her  mother's  forehead,  where  the  wave  of 
dark  hair  rolled  away,  contrasting  with  the  white  skin.  On  the 
twenty- first  day  her  mother  and  grandmother  noticed  her  steady 
gaze  at  a  black  silk  dress  crossed  with  white  stripes. 

The  twenty-fifth  day  I  observed  convulsive  shutting  of  the  eyes 
against  dazzling  light,  or  against  moderate  light  after  sleep  or  after 
darkness,  exactly  as  described  by  Preyer;  at  moderate  light  under 
other  conditions  the  baby  gazed  with  apparent  comfort. 

In  the  fifth  week,  twenty-ninth  day,  she  first  went  outdoors; 
she  seemed  to  dislike  the  light,  and  kept  her  eyes  shut. 

After  this,  pleasure  in  gazing  at  bright  surfaces  was  gradually 
displaced  by  interest  in  faces.  In  the  sixth  week,  however,  the 
baby  gazed  a  good  deal  at  the  angle  of  the  ceiling  and  wall  where  a 
dark  border  joined  the  light  ceiling.  In  the  seventh  week  (forty- 
seventh  day)  she  stopped  in  the  midst  of  fretting  to  gaze  through  an 
open  window  at  white  clothes  hanging  in  the  sunlight. 

In  the  twenty-fifth  week  she  was  evidently  interested  in  pro- 
ducing a  change  of  light  in  playing  "peekaboo;"  when  a  handker- 
(  10) 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  11 

chief  was  thrown  over  her  face  she  would  first  stare  about  the  room 
through  it  some  seconds,  then  pull  it  off. 

In  the  second  year,  the  difference  of  light  and  darkness  was  a 
matter  of  a  good  deal  of  interest.  In  the  twentieth  month,  590th 
day,  I  saw  her  outdoors,  especially  when  driving,  cover  her  eyes 
several  times  with  her  hands.  I  thought  the  sunlight  might  be  too 
brilliant,  but  it  is  more  likely  she  was  experimenting,  for  in  the  fol- 
lowing weeks  she  would  often  cover  her  eyes  with  her  hands  and 
take  them  away;  hide  her  face  in  a  cushion,  or  on  her  own  arms, 
often  saying,  "Dark!"  then  lookup,  —  "Light  now!"  The  598th 
day,  on  waking,  her  first  remark  was  "  Light.  Aunty  make  light," 
pointing  to  a  place  on  the  wall  where  the  light  must  have  fallen 
from  a  concealed  candle  I  had  lit  in  the  night.  The  611th  day  I 
was  told  she  pointed  to  a  mass  of  dark  clouds  hanging  low,  and 
said,  "  Dark  over  there."  The  same  day,  after  hearing  a  story 
about  the  moon  that  interested  her  much  in  the  twentieth  and 
twenty-first  months,  she  asked  over  and  over,  "  Dark?"  Answered, 
"No,  it  was  light  after  the  moon  came,"  she  would  say,  "Light 
now?"  or  "  Dark?  No."  She  then  buried  her  face  in  a  cushion; 
then  looked  under  the  cushion,  saying  softly,  "  Dark,"  then  down 
behind  the  lounge,  saying,  "Dark  there."  When  a  curtain  was 
raised  higher,  producing  a  change  in  the  light,  she  would  comment 
at  once,  "  Light."  I  was  told  that  on  a  railroad  trip,  641st  day,  she 
made  no  comment  on  entering  tunnels,  but  regularly  ejaculated,  on 
emerging,  "Light!" 

In  the  twenty-third  month,  699th  day,  the  moonlit  garden,  seen 
from  the  window,  seemed  to  be  quite  a  strange  place  to  her;  she 
did  not  recognize  objects,  and  when  I  spoke  of  the  lilies,  e.g., 
would  say,  "Where  lilies?" 

In  the  twenty-fourth  month,  720th  day,  she  saw  heat  lightning. 
When  she  had  been  told  its  name,  she  ran  from  one  member  of  the 
family  to  another,  saying  earnestly,  "Ruth  saw  flash  lightning!" — 
"What  did  it  look  like?"  she  was  asked.  She  shut  her  eyes  as 
tightly  as  she  could,  and  clinched  her  hands,  saying,  "  Looked  like 
just  this  way." 


12  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 


2.    Movements  of  Lids. 

I  watched  for  the  various  asymmetric  movements  mentioned  by 
Preyer,  but  no  one  about  my  niece  was  able  to  see  as  much  asym- 
metry as  in  the  case  of  his  child.  The  lids  were  never  but  twice  — ■ 
on  the  53d  and  54th  days  —  unequally  raised  to  any  extent  that  was 
perceptible  without  close  scrutiny;  both  times  were  immediately 
after  sleep. 

On  the  27th  day  her  mother  saw  the  lids  raised  with  a  downward 
look,  just  as  noted  by  Preyer;  and  for  some  days  after  I  saw  this 
repeatedly  myself,  the  sclerotic  coat  visible  above  the  iris;  I  have 
no  note  of  it  except  when  she  was  in  the  bath,  at  which  time  the 
movements  of  her  eyes  were  more  active  and  more  asymmetric. 
After  the  first  month  this  look  was  rare,  noted  not  at  all  in  the  sec- 
ond month,  but  several  times  at  the  end  of  the  third  and  during  the 
first  half  of  the  fourth,  90th  to  105th  days;  so  far  as  my  notes  indi- 
cate, it  was  always  when  the  baby  was  being  bathed,  or  wiped  after 
her  bath. 

On  the  104th  day  I  first  saw  the  brow  wrinkled  in  looking 
upward;  but  for  some  days  before,  the  skin  between  the  forehead 
and  fontanel  had  wrinkled  with  this  look. 

Contrary  to  Preyer's  observation,  she  nursed  from  the  first  with 
eyes  closed ;  after  the  47th  day,  the  lids  were  dropped  oftener  than 
entirely  closed;  I  never  but  twice  saw  them  open,  and  on  one  of 
these  occasions  it  was  because  she  caught  sight  of  a  candle  as  she 
was  placed  at  the  breast,  and  stared  at  it  awhile  before  closing  her 
eyes.  I  have  since  watched  and  inquired  about  several  other  babies, 
and  found  none  that  nursed  with  open  eyes. 

We  were  not  able  to  observe  any  regular  relation  between  widely 
opened  eyes  and  pleasure  in  the  case  of  my  niece.  Up  to  the  fifth 
week  her  eyes  at  no  time  opened  as  widely  as  they  did  from  that 
date;  and  it  was  not  till  the  seventh  week  that  I  noticed  them 
stretched  widely,  in  the  bath  and  while  wiped,  in  connection  with 
signs  of  satisfaction, —  panting,  and  movements  of  limbs.  She  had 
then  had  instead  for  a  couple  of  weeks  a  habit  when  bathed,  or 
undressed  by  the  fire,  of  fixing  her  eyes  on  her  mother's  face  and 
turning    her    head   away,  so  as    to  give  a  curious  sidelong    look, 


sniNN.i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  13 

which  somehow  had  an  unmistakable  effect  of  high  satisfaction. 
Up  to  the  45th  day  the  look  appeared  only  when  she  was  bathed  or 
undressed ;  on  that  day  once  when  lying  dressed,  but  very  comfort- 
able, on  the  lounge;  after  this  I  have  no  more  note  of  it;  it  seems 
to  have  disappeared  as  an  expression  of  satisfaction  with  the  ap- 
pearance of  the  wide-eyed  staring,  — which,  however,  never  became 
a  marked  habit.  I  never  saw  her  shut  her  eyes  under  discomfort, 
as  having  nose  or  ears  washed;  she  either  submitted  indifferently, 
or  expressed  discomfort  by  wriggling  and  uttering  little  sounds. 
The  fallen  lids  when  nursing  accompanied  an  expression  of  great 
content.     In  fear  and  in  surprise  her  eyes  were  opened  wide. 

The  first  time  that  she  winked  at  having  a  head  suddenly  thrust 
close  to  her  eyes  was  on  the  56th  day,  when  the  wink  followed 
slowly  but  regularly,  six  times  without  a  failure.  It  had  been  tried 
in  vain  almost  daily  for  weeks  before.  I  have  noted  several  in- 
stances of  winking  at  sudden  sounds.  139th  day  when  someone 
snapped  his  teeth  together  several  times  a  foot  or  so  from  her  face, 
she  winked  eveiy  time,  and  looked  much  surprised;  167th  day  I 
was  told  of  her  winking  at  the  wind;  185th  day,  banging  her  rattle 
down  in  high  glee,  she  winked  at  every  blow.  As  a  general  thing 
she  was  not  easily  startled,  and  I  saw  very  few  noticeable  instances 
of  winking.  I  did  not  watch  for  them  carefully,  however,  knowing 
that  one  should  be  a  physiologist  to  observe  intelligently  the  reflex 
actions. 

3.    Movements  of  Balls. 

These  also  were  generally  symmetric ;  but  occasionally  in  the 
first  and  second  months,  especially  the  first,  there  was  a  distinct 
crossing,  and  several  times  in  the  third  month  a  slight  one.  When 
in  the  bath  she  rolled  her  eyes  about  more  irregularly  than  at  any 
other  time.  Notwithstanding  that  the  movements  of  her  eyes  were 
generally  in  unison,  there  was  about  them  always  in  the  first  two 
months  a  certain  appearance  of  convergence,  —  as  in  the  case  of 
all  babies  of  this  age  I  have  noticed;  and  this  look  was  occasionally 
seen  as  late  as  the  ninth  month,  when  my  last  note  of  it  occurs, 
249th  day. 


14  University  of  California.  [Vol.  %. 


Fixation. 

I  have  noted  her  staring  at  faces  especially,  among  light  surfaces, 
during  the  third  week;  this  increased  in  the  fourth.  On  the  25th 
day,  as  she  lay  wide  awake  and  comfortable  in  her  grandmother's 
lap,  staring  thus  at  her  face,  with  an  appearance  of  attention,  I  leaned 
down  close  beside,  so  as  to  bring  my  face  into  the  field  of  vision. 
The  baby  turned  her  eyes  (not  head)  and  gazed  at  my  face  with  the 
same  appearance  of  attention,  even  effort,  in  slight  tension  of  brows 
and  lips;  then  back  to  her  grandmother's  face,  again  to  mine,  so 
several  times.  This  seems  clearly  Preyer's  "second  stage"  of  fixa- 
tion. At  last  she  seemed  to  become  aware  of  my  red  gown,  or  the 
lamplight  striking  the  shoulder,  and  not  only  moved  her  eyes,  but 
threw  her  head  far  back  to  look  at  my  shoulder,  with  a  new  expres- 
sion, a  sort  of  dim  interest,  or  eagerness. 

The  "third  stage,"  the  following  of  an  object  in  motion,  I  did 
not  fix  satisfactorily.  The  nurse,  who  was  a  careful  observer,  said 
that  the  baby  followed  the  motion  of  her  hand  on  the  9th  day.  I 
could  not  satisfy  myself  that  she  did,  even  as  late  as  the  fifth  week; 
her  eyes  seemed  sometimes  briefly  to  follow  the  moving  hand,  but 
she  was  so  active,  moving  head  and  eyes  constantly,  that  I  could 
not  trust  the  appearance;  her  mother  was  satisfied  that  she  followed 
the  motion.  On  the  33d  day  I  tried  a  candle,  and  her  eyes  followed 
it  unmistakably,  rolling  as  far  as  they  could,  and  then  the  head  was 
turned  to  follow  still  farther.  Had  I  tried  a  luminous  object  earlier, 
I  might  have  found  that  she  could  follow  it.  I  find  that  almost  all 
mothers  and  nurses  place  the  attainment  of  power  to  follow  a  mov- 
ing object  much  earlier  than  this,  usually  in  the  first  week. 

In  this  same  week,  the  fifth,  she  aquired  the  habit  mentioned 
above  of  fixing  a  sidelong  look  on  her  mother. 

In  the  fifth  week,  too,  when  held  up  against  the  shoulder,  she 
would  straighten  up  her  head  to  see  around;  and  thereafter  looking 
about,  as  if  to  see  what  she  could  see,  became  more  and  more  her 
habit,  and  together  with  gazing  at  faces,  was  her  chief  occupation 
till  grasping  was  established.  By  the  tenth  week,  she  would  turn 
her  head  to  look  about  thus. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  15 

On  the  40th  day,  her  eyes  for  the  first  time  followed  the  move- 
ment of  a  person  (who  had  possibly  attracted  her  attention  by  a 
voice  and  appearance  novel  to  her)  as  he  moved  slowly  in  a  semi- 
circle about  the  knee  where  she  lay. 

At  the  close  of  the  eighth  week,  on  the  56th  day,  as  she  lay  on 
her  mother's  lap  by  the  fire  and  I  sat  close  by,  she  gazed  fixedly  at 
my  face  some  fifteen  minutes  without  removing  her  eyes;  indeed, 
as  her  mother  turned  her,  in  undressing  and  rubbing,  she  screwed 
her  head  around  comically  to  keep  her  eyes  fixed  on  me.  At  last 
she  turned  her  head  clear  over  and  looked  at  her  mother's  face. 
Her  mother  turned  her  again  toward  mine,  which  she  surveyed  for 
a  time,  then  again  turned  and  looked  at  her  mother's;  all  this  with 
serious  attention  and  effort.  Whether  she  had  arrived  at  a  clearer 
focusing  of  our  faces  than  before,  and  was  interested  in  it,  or  whether 
this  incident  was  the  dawn  of  recognition  of  us  as  separate  persons, 
I  cannot  tell.  It  was  the  same  day  that  she  had  first  responded  by 
a  wink  to  a  threat  at  her  eyes. 

Soon  after,  60th  day,  I  saw  some  indication  that  she  recognized 
her  mother,  for  she  stopped  fretting  when  hungry  on  seeing  her 
come  in  at  a  door  in  her  line  of  vision,  not  three  feet  away;  no  cer- 
tain indication,  however,  for  anyone's  entrance  might  have  diverted 
her  attention.  The  first  unmistakable  recognition  by  sight  alone 
was  on  the  80th  day,  when  she  smiled  and  gave  a  joyous  cry  on 
seeing  her  grandfather  enter.  She  certainly  knew  her  mother  before 
this,  but  whether  by  sight  alone,  or  by  the  aid  of  hearing  and  touch, 
I  could  not  tell ;  and  though  during  the  next  six  weeks  she  showed 
in  many  small  ways  that  she  distinguished  the  persons  about  her 
one  from  another,  I  could  not  get  proof  that  it  was  by  sight  only. 
For  instance,  when  I  came  in  in  the  morning  and  spoke  to  her,  she 
was  accustomed  to  greet  me  with  smile  or  cry;  on  the  167th  day, 
I  came  in  without  speaking,  to  see  if  she  would  make  the  same 
demonstration ;  she  looked  at  me  seriously,  then  fixed  her  gaze  on 
a  lamp  I  held;  I  set  it  away,  then  came  and  spoke  to  her;  but  she 
made  no  response  till  I  bent  down,  then  caught  my  face  familiarly 
in  her  hands.  By  such  vague  behavior,  she  defeated  efforts  to 
establish  certainly  her  discrimination  of  our  faces  until  long  after  I 
was  sure  that  she  did  know  them.  Late  in  the  fourth  month,  she 
noticed   any  change    in  our  appearance  wrought  by  headgear  or 


1 6  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

wraps,  with  surprised  looks  at  the  articles,  however  unobtrusive  in 
form  and  color;  and  on  the  160th  day,  though  staring  with  the 
usual  surprise  at  our  hats,  she  paid  no  attention  to  that  of  a  stran- 
ger who  had  come  in  with  us.  As  she  would  answer  a  smile  by 
laughter  and  movements;  once  laughed  at  a  grimace  (130th  day); 
in  the  sixteenth  week,  when  especially  interested  in  an  uncle,  would 
smile  when  he  looked  at  her;  and  (e.g.,  119th  day)  watched  our 
lips,  even  looking  from  her  mother's  lips  to  mine,  when  interested 
in  a  sound  we  made, — it  seems  certain  that  by  the  fourth  month  at 
least,  our  faces  must  have  been  clear  to  her.  In  the  sixth  month, 
being  separated  from  her  mother  when  hungry,  she  cried  hard  for 
her,  watching  at  every  turn  in  a  corridor  and  every  door  passed 
through,  and  was  instantly  comforted  at  sight  of  her;  but  I  had  no 
dqubt  she  had  recognized  her  perfectly  weeks  before. 

The  141st  day,  she  seemed  to  recognize  her  grandmother 
through  the  window;  but  the  day  before,  I  was  told,  this  slight 
obstacle  had  prevented  recognition  till  her  grandmother  raised  the 
sash  and  smiled  at  her  alternately  with  and  without  its  interven- 
tion; and  on  the  143d  day  she  seemed  to  find  me  unfamiliar  seen 
through  the  pane.  On  the  158th  day  she  held  out  her  arms  to  me 
when  she  was  some  ten  feet  from  the  closed  window  through  which 
I  looked  at  her;  and  in  the  eighth  month,  228th  day,  she  recog- 
nized her  grandfather's  head  some  thirty  feet  away,  through  two 
closed  windows.  Yet  as  late  as  the  twentieth  month,  being  in  a 
company  of  strangers  in  a  room  not  strongly  lighted,  she  took  them 
for  neighbors,  calling  one  and  another  by  the  names  of  persons  to 
whom  they  bore  slight  resemblance. 

Once  in  the  fifth  month,  136th  day,  I  called  to  her,  the  back  of 
a  willow  chair  being  between  us.  She  looked  each  side  of  the  chair 
with  growing  surprise,  and  at  last  directly  at  the  back  several  times, 
but  without  seeing  me,  though  it  was  easy  to  see  through  the  open 
work:  either  she  could  not  recognize  my  face  crossed  by  the  wil- 
low rods,  or  did  not  understand  carrying  her  look  past  the  obstacle; 
her  face  was  somewhat  troubled  as  well  as  amazed,  jaw  dropped 
and  brows  lifted.1 


1  Professor  Joseph  Le  Conte  reminds  me  that  if  she  looked  at  the  chair  back 
she  saw  my  face  doubled. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  17 

The  objects  early  noticed  by  her,  other  than  bright  surfaces  or 
shining  points,  and  faces,  were  (if  I  pass  by  the  lighted  surfaces  of 
color,  25th  and  37th  days):  42c!  day,  bunch  of  yellow  flowers;  56th 
and  57th,  knot  of  red  ribbon;  62d,  red  and  yellow  strips;  68th, 
bunch  of  bright  sachets,  shadows  of  chandelier  quivering  on  the 
ceiling;  80th  day,  she  became  silent  when  fretting,  and  watched 
attentively  the  leaves  of  a  small  notebook  fluttered  before  her,  mov- 
ing hands  and  feet  with  interest;  87th,  an  eyeglass  dangled  from  a 
string.  Up  to  nearly  three  months  old,  therefore,  she  had  not,  so 
far  as  I  saw,  in  spite  of  her  looking  about,  really  looked  at  any  spe- 
cial objects  that  were  not  lighted  up,  colored,  or  in  motion.  On 
the  87th  day,  however,  she  twice  looked  seriously,  but  without 
appearance  of  curiosity,  at  a  rattle  in  her  hand;  after  this,  fixing  her 
eyes  with  attention  on  various  objects  became  common.  Bright  or 
colored  ones  did  not  seem  to  keep  their  ascendency  after  the  third 
or  at  all  events  the  fourth  month,  nor  moving  ones  whose  motion 
was  not  followed,  though  they  continued  to  attract:  105th  day  she 
watched  the  moving  landscape  from  the  car  window  for  a  half  hour 
with  pleasure;  130th  day  watched  the  windmill;  i62d,  noticed 
especially  of  all  the  sights  in  the  city,  passing  cable  cars,  walls  of 
the  elevator  as  it  rose  and  sank,  and  trees  blowing  outside  a  win- 
dow. 

I  had  noted  especially  that  up  to  the  incident  of  the  rattle  on 
the  87th  day  she  had  never  looked  at  anything  held  in  her  hands, 
even  when  she  showed  interest  in  having  it  there;  and  thereafter  she 
looked  at  such  objects  rarely  and  with  slight  attention,  till  grasp- 
ing was  fully  established.  This  process  was  gradually  acquired 
during  the  whole  of  the  fourth  month;  I  did  not  see  her  fix  an 
object  with  her  eye  and  then  try  to  reach  it  before  the  1 13th  day. 
There  was  a  constant  progress  in  co-ordination  of  eye  and  touch 
in  learning  this.  I  shall  hope  hereafter  to  give  details  of  this  under 
the  subject  of  Grasping.  I  never  but  once  saw  her  hold  up  an 
object,  —  her  rattle,  as  she  lay  on  her  back  in  the  sun,  134th  day, 
—  and  inspect  it  long  and  carefully;  nor  did  she  ever  at  any  time 
look  with  attention  at  her  own  hands,  as  some  babies  do ;  not  even 
when,  97th  day,  she  was  pounding  her  fist  down  hard  on  the  table, 
did  she  look  at  it. 

As  to  the  size  of  objects  of  which  she  had  clear  enough  vision 


1 8  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

to  try  to  grasp  them:  the  rattle,  an  unusually  small  one,  was  the 
smallest  I  have  note  of  until  the  latter  part  of  the  fifth  month,  when 
she  would  reach  for,  seize,  and  play  with  the  curtain  cord,  and  put 
the  knot  into  her  mouth;  in  the  sixth  month,  158th  day,  she  tried 
to  catch  flies  on  the  pane;  168th,  tried  to  reach  a  fragment  of  red 
sugar  on  the  floor,  scarcely  one-fourth  inch  in  diameter,  and  there- 
after several  times  bits  of  paper,  petals,  etc.,  of  similar  size.  In  the 
sixth  month  also  a  few  other  very  small  objects  were  grasped,  as  a 
rubber  ring,  of  the  smallest  size  used  for  papers,  and  strings  were 
favorites.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  eighth  month,  the  smallest  scraps 
and  shreds  on  the  carpet,  down  to  pin-head  size,  occupied  her  a 
good  deal;  once  a  single  hair. 

I  have  mentioned  that  she  followed  the  motion  of  a  person  in 
the  second  month.  I  did  not  myself  see  any  advance  in  this  power 
in  the  third,  but  was  told  that  on  the  84th  day  she  watched  her 
grandfather  out  of  the  room  when  he  quitted  her  after  a  play,  kept 
her  eyes  on  him  as  he  stood  in  the  next  room  a  few  seconds,  and 
gave  a  joyous  crow  when  he  turned  to  come  back.  By  the  fifteenth 
week,  she  followed  moving  persons  constantly  with  her  eyes,  and 
thereafter  my  notes  of  it  are  frequent.  By  the  nineteenth  week  she 
was  fond  of  following  them  through  long  processes,  as  setting  the 
table,  without  a  deviation  of  attention.  By  the  twenty-fourth,  she 
would  sometimes  follow  the  movements  of  hands:  e.  g.,  she  would 
watch  closely  the  motion  of  forks  and  spoons  to  our  lips  at  table. 

Meanwhile,  in  the  fourth  month,  1 19th  day,  she  once  followed  a 
bunch  of  sachets  with  her  eyes  as  it  was  swung  back  and  forth  be- 
fore her  for  many  minutes.  In  the  nineteenth  week,  129th  day,  she 
once  looked  after  a  napkin  ring  on  dropping  it;  but  it  was  a  solitary 
occurrence,  and  she  did  not  begin  to  look  at  all  intelligently  after 
falling  objects  till  the  next  month,  the  160th  day;  she  could  in  the 
nineteenth  week  find  her  rattle  if  she  dropped  it  on  the  tray  of  the 
high-chair  immediately  before  her,  but  not  in  her  lap  or  on  the  floor. 
After  the  160th  day  for  about  a  week,  she  dropped  articles  on  pur- 
pose to  watch  them,  sometimes  for  as  long  as  anyone  would  pick 
them  up,  or  would  hold  her  out  to  pick  them  up  herself.  She  had 
another  period  of  earnest  and  persistent  experiment  in  this  direction 
in  the  eighth  month. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  19 

The  136th  day  she  watched  steam  from  the  kettle,  without  try- 
ing to  follow  its  motion;  158th,  trying  to  catch  flies  on  the  pane, 
looked  up  after  them  when  they  went  too  high  for  her;  i62d, 
watched  objects  from  the  train  window,  and  would  now  and  then 
look  back  to  keep  her  eyes  on  one  as  the  train  moved. 

Early  in  the  seventh  month  she  would  follow  the  flight  of  the 
pigeons  as  they  flew  up  near  by;  no  smaller  bird  till  the  230th  day 
I  was  told  she  followed  a  blackbird;  about  the  middle  of  the  tenth 
month  she  began  to  follow  the  flight  of  small  birds  if  her  attention 
was  drawn  by  their  twittering,  or  if  she  chanced  to  be  looking  as 
they  flew  up:  in  the  eleventh  month,  321st  day  and  thereafter,  she 
followed  the  flitrht  of  large,  slow  butterflies. 

As  to  the  distance  at  which  she  seemed  to  see  things,  I  have 
mentioned  her  watching  her  grandfather  into  the  next  room,  some 
fifteen  feet  away,  on  the  84th  day;  by  the  fifteenth  week  she  often 
noticed  us  and  smiled  at  us  across  the  room;  in  the  seventeenth 
(1 15th  day),  at  her  mother  once  in  the  next  room,  fifteen  or  twenty 
feet  away.  I  thought  in  the  seventeenth  week  that  she  showed  a 
special  interest  and  a  sort  of  curiosity  over  faces  at  that  distance, 
and  wondered  if  they  looked  to  her  conspicuously  smaller. 

The  objects  that  she  would  watch  through  the  window  in  the 
fifth  month  were  usually  within  fifty  feet  of  the  window.  The  1 56th 
day  I  was  told  she  watched  me  as  I  stepped  into  a  phaeton  and 
until  it  passed  out  of  sight  behind  trees,  perhaps  one  hundred  feet 
away.  The  same  day  she  watched  her  mother  go  some  twenty 
yards  away  to  gather  flowers.  She  never  reached  arms  to  seize  ob- 
jects much  out  of  her  reach, —  never  more  than  say  three  feet  away ; 
if  one  held  arms  to  her  from  a  distance,  however,  she  would  respond, 
laughing;  but  this,  I  think,  was  merely  a  gesture,  without  expecta- 
tion that  we  would  take  her  from  that  distance.  In  the  sixth  month, 
she  had  no  difficulty  in  recognizing  our  faces  at  upper  windows. 
On  the  175th  day  she  seemed  to  be  watching  a  team  passing  about 
one  hundred  feet  from  the  window,  she  herself  being  some  eight  or 
ten  feet  from  the  window,  in  the  room.  Early  in  the  ninth  month, 
279th  day,  she  noticed  through  a  closed  window  a  team  some  seventy- 
five  feet  away ;  296th  day  she  recognized  my  face  at  an  upper  win- 
dow when  she  was  about  forty  feet  from  the  house,  but  she  also 


20  University  of  California.  [Vou  i. 

heard  my  voice.  On  the  293d  she  called  for  a  red  rose  seen  at  a 
distance  of  perhaps  eighty  feet  through  trees  and  bushes,  and  kept 
her  eye  on  it  all  the  way  as  she  was  carried  to  it,  jubilating  as  she 
saw  she  was  to  have  it. 

I  saw  no  indication  in  the  first  year  that  she  ever  looked  really 
away,  into  the  distance;  but  the  clay  she  was  a  year  old  she  proved 
to  know  the  moon,  pointed  out  to  her  by  someone  three  days  earlier. 
After  this  she  was  taken  to  the  window  to  see  it,  or  outdoors  when 
the  weather  was  warm  enough,  every  full  moon  until  the  days  grew 
too  long,  about  six  months  (as  her  birth  was  on  October  6).  In  the 
fifty-fifth  week,  her  attention  was  called  to  a  star,  and  thereafter  she 
was  interested  in  looking  for  stars  also,  and  would  sometimes  detect 
them  very  quickly  and  point,  crying  "  'Tar."  She  took  great  pleasure 
in  looking  at  moon  and  stars,  and  would  greet  them  with  a  shout 
of  joy. 

Meanwhile,  in  the  latter  part  of  the  thirteenth  month,  she  began 
to  recognize  cows  a  half-mile  away  on  the  hills,  not,  however,  from 
her  knowledge  of  the  appearance  of  a  cow  near  by,  but  because 
they  had  been  pointed  out  to  her  a  little  while  before  from  a  car 
window. 

It  was  late  in  the  eighteenth  month,  542d  day,  before  she  said 
anything  that  showed  an  idea  that  the  moon  could  be  reached;  she 
then  asked,  "Ea'?  Woo?" — "O,  no,  Ruth  can't  eat  the  moon." 
"Man?" — "No,  men  cannot  eat  it."  "Laly?" — "No,  nor  ladies." 
"Owgu?" — "No,  aunty  cannot;  it  is  up  in  the  sky;  it  is  too  far  to 
reach."  The  words,  "too  far,"  and  "reach,"  were  quite  familiar 
to  her  before,  in  connection  with  things  hung  just  beyond  her  grasp, 
and  the  like,  but  this  seemed  to  be  her  first  idea  of  great  distance, 
and  she  was  interested  and  curious;  she  stretched  out  her  arm  and 
cried,"  Rea'!  Far!"  over  and  over.  A  week  later  she  began  asking 
me  to  get  the  moon.  When  told  that  we  could  not  reach  it,  she  de- 
sired to  be  shown  that  we  could  not,  and  wished  each  of  us  in  turn 
t<>  stretch  an  arm  to  show  her  how  far  our  best  efforts  fell  short. 
When  we  went  into  the  house,  and  she  was  told  to  tell  grandma 
what  she  had  seen,  she  answered,  as  usual,  "Moon,"  then  added, 
"Far!"  Next  morning  at  breakfast,  hearing  us  speak  of  her  wish 
to  get  the  moon,  she  turned  in  her  chair,  and  reached  her  hand 
prettily  toward  the  ceiling,  looking  up  as  if  far  away,  and  crying, 
"Far!"     This  she  did  several  times. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  21 

Up  to  the  fourth  six  months,  she  certainly  did  not  distinguish  at 
as  great  a  distance  as  we;  we  are  all  rather  far-sighted,  however. 
From  the  seventy-second  to  the  seventy-fifth  week  she  repeatedly 
misnamed  men  or  boys  at  perhaps  twenty  yards  distance ;  the  less 
familiar  person  being  almost  always  called  by  the  name  of  the  one 
better  known.  Any  man  in  farmer's  clothes  at  that  distance  would 
be  called  by  the  name  of  her  uncle;  any  boy  of  eight  or  ten,  "  Ray;" 
any  child  under  four  or  five,  "Harrison."  As  the  person  mistaken 
came  nearer,  if  he  was  known  to  her,  she  would  correct  herself;  if 
a  stranger,  she  would  say,  "Man!"  or  "Boy!"  or  "Baby!" — always 
with  an  air  slightly  surprised  and  crestfallen.  She  once  mistook  a 
strange  young  girl  at  a  distance  for  one  slightly  known  to  her;  but 
never  made  any  error  about  women  she  knew  at  all  well,  perhaps 
because  their  garments  made  them  recognizable  farther  away,  per- 
haps because  she  really  knew  them  better  than  the  men  of  the 
family.  On  the  590th  day,  again,  seeing  her  uncle  (a  tall  man) 
about  a  hundred  feet  away,  she  called  him  by  the  name  of  a  half- 
grown  Portuguese  boy,  who  worked  about  the  place  and  was  well 
known  by  sight  to  her  ;  there  was  no  resemblance  in  face,  and  none 
perceptible  to  us  in  gait  or  bearing.  As  he  came  nearer,  she  be- 
came doubtful,  but  did  not  call  her  uncle's  name  positively  till  he 
was  within  fifty  feet.  She  looked  at  him  very  intently  then,  and 
later  in  the  same  day  recognized  him  at  once  when  seen  more  than 
one  hundred  feet  away.  This  goes  to  show  that  her  mistakes  were 
due  not  so  much  to  defect  in  distant  vision  as  to  failure  to  observe 
and  fix  in  mind  those  general  aspects  by  which  we  recognize  per- 
sons at  a  distance.  In  this  case,  a  chief  element  of  error  was  failure 
to  estimate  size;  but  not  in  the  earlier  instances.  I  never  noticed 
an  error  in  distant  recognition  afterward.  The  626th  day,  however, 
seeing  her  grandfather  occupied  some  twenty-five  feet  away,  she 
said  he  was  "eating,"  though  he  was  in  fact  counting  money,  every 
outline  and  motion  clearly  defined  against  a  window;  and  she  could 
not  make  out  what  he  was  doing  till  she  had  come  close  to  him. 
On  the  645th  day,  again,  I  was  told  that  she  mistook  a  white  cow 
on  a  distant  hillside  for  a  goat,  —  the  estimate  of  size  here  again 
evidently  at  fault;  and  quite  recently,  late  in  the  third  year,  she 
called  horses  far  away  on  the  hills  pigs.1 

1  The  following  notes,  from  records  kept  by  Mrs.  Eleanor  Sharpe,  of  San 


22  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 


5.   Direction. 

On  the  87th  day  I  first  saw  her  \ooV  for  something:  she  was 
much  interested  in  a  guest,  a  lively  girl,  and  not  only  followed  her 
movements,  but  would  look  for  her  when  out  of  sight  (89th  day). 
93d  day,  just  before  three  months  old,  she  turned  her  head  and 
looked  with  an  appearance  of  intention  in  the  direction  of  a  sound 
heard  a  few  seconds  before.  I  had  been  told  of  her  doing  this 
several  times  in  this  and  the  preceding  week,  once  at  the  snap- 
ping of  the  fire;  but  could  not  get  her  to  look  by  any  sound  I  could 
make,  though  she  would  show  attention  by  her  manner.  Thereafter 
she  would  sometimes  turn  her  head  to  look  for  the  source  of  a 
sound,  but  never  at  all  regularly.  On  the  I22d  day  she  leaned  first 
to  one  side  then  the  other,  to  look  around  the  chair  back  with  quick, 
eager  turns  of  her  head,  to  see  her  uncle  playing;  when  I  took  her 
to  his  side,  she  watched  his  fingers  eagerly,  and  when  he  began  to 
sing,  gazed  alternately  up  to  his  face  and  down  at  his  fingers,  throw- 
ing her  head  far  back  to  look  up,  and  alternating  her  gaze  about 
every  five  seconds  during  the  entire  stanza. 

Her  looking  after  objects  dropped  has  been  mentioned  above. 
I32d  day  she  looked  repeatedly  to  see  what  touched  the  back  of 
her  head,  when  she  had  bent  it  back  till  it  touched  the  floor. 

Francisco,  and  by  Mrs.  Mabel   Beatty  (B.  L.,  University  of  California),  and 
kindly  placed  at  my  disposal,  bear  on  the  subjects  of  Fixation  and  Direction  : — 

I.  Before  the  child  was  a  month  old,  he  turned  eyes  and  head  to  follow  a 
lighted  car  down  the  street. 

In  the  third  month  he  was  interested  in  watching  his  brothers  at  their  play. 

In  the  fourth  month,  106th  day,  he  did  not  know  his  nurse  when  she  was 
dressed  to  go  out,  and  was  on  the  point  of  crying  until  she  spoke  to  him. 

In  the  tenth  month,  when  in  his  bath,  he  would  try  to  grasp  the  sponge 
or  the  stream  of  water  from  it,  if  held  within  reach,  but  never  if  much  beyond 
reach. 

II.  In  the  third  month  the  child  seemed  to  be  studying  faces. 

In  first  learning  to  grasp,  in  the  17th  and  iSth  weeks,  he  did  not  always  hit 
accurately. 

In  the  23d  week,  he  looked  for  a  cable  car  in  the  proper  direction. 

In  the  sixth  month  he  began  to  follow  the  direction  of  a  pointing  finger; 
when  told  to  look  at  anything,  he  would  first  look  to  see  the  pointing  finger,  then 
fix  the  object;  yet  as  late  as  the  eighth  month  he  would  sometimes  look  only  at 
the  finger.  In  the  tenth  month  he  began  to  point  himself,  indicating  the  direc- 
tion in  which  he  wished  to  go. 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  23 

On  the  133d  day  occurred  a  rather  striking  incident.  I  had 
been  holding  her,  but  gave  her  to  her  mother  to  put  to  sleep;  she 
was  no  sooner  settled  in  her  mother's  lap  than  she  began  to  turn 
her  head  to  watch  me;  and  when  her  mother  turned  to  prevent 
this,  she  would  screw  her  head  over  her  shoulder;  so  I  rose  from 
my  seat  to  the  right  of  her,  and  crossing  toward  the  left,  sat  down 
concealed.  She  did  not  follow  the  motion,  but  after  a  few  seconds 
raised  her  head  from  her  mother's  arm,  and  screwing  it  about, 
searched  the  farthest  /r/7-hand  quarter  of  the  room,  and  it  was  some 
time  before  she  would  be  contented  without  seeing  me  there.  She 
seemed  able  to  infer  from  the  direction  in  which  I  had  passed  from 
her  field  of  vision  that  I  was  to  be  looked  for  somewhere  on  an 
extension  of  that  line.  Yet  she  did  not,  as  a  rule,  show  a  sense  of 
direction  nearly  so  good:  see  Interpretation,  below. 

Her  earliest  ability  to  direct  her  look,  except  when  it  was  drawn 
along  by  something  it  was  following,  was  when  guided  by  a  con- 
tinuous sound  close  by;  from  the  fifth  to  the  seventh  week,  she 
had  stared  into  the  face  of  a  person  striking  the  piano,  as  if  the 
sound  came  thence;  on  the  45th  day,  she  turned  once  and  looked 
at  the  keys.     On  the  57th  day  she  began  to  watch  the  keys. 

In  the  twenty-third  week  she  would  look  around  her  on  the 
floor  for  playthings  she  had  dropped. 

The  165  th  day  I  looked  at  her  and  called,  over  the  top  of  a  tall 
screen,  and  withdrew  my  head  and  reappeared  several  times.  She 
would  watch  the  spot  in  the  interval, —  a  few  seconds.  She  had  no 
difficulty  in  looking  directly  at  the  place  whence  the  voice  came, 
though  it  was  so  high  I  had  to  stand  on  tiptoe  on  a  chair,  and  no 
one  had  ever  spoken  to  her  thence.  Three  days  later  I  tried  it 
again,  appearing  at  different  spots,  several  feet  apart,  along  the  long 
screen.  She  had  no  trouble  in  looking  at  me  at  once,  though  she 
became  a  little  confused  as  to  which  spot  to  watch  for  my  reappear- 
ance, and  if  I  did  not  appear  promptly  at  the  last  one,  would  go 
back  and  watch  the  first.  About  the  same  date,  I  called  to  her 
from  an  upper  window;  she  looked  assiduously,  but  never  high 
enough,  and  grew  troubled  and  surprised;  her  mother  tried  to 
make  her  follow  her  [jointing  hand,  but  she  did  not  get  the  idea  of 
following  on  in  the  direction  indicated,  farther  than  the  hand.  At 
last  her  grandmother  rustled  a  paper  sharply,  which  somehow  drew 
her  eyes  up  to  the  window,  twelve  or  thirteen  feet  above  her.     In 


24  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

one  or  two  other  trials  we  found  that  a  sufficiently  sharp  or  strik- 
ing sound  somehow  enabled  her  to  locate  the  source.  When  she 
discovered  us,  she  apparently  knew  us  at  once,  smiling  and  moving 
her  arms.  Although  ordinarily  so  accurate  in  locating  a  voice, 
occasionally  she  would  look  at  the  wrong  person  when  two  were 
close  together. 

I  noticed  in  this  week  that  her  attention  could  be  drawn  in  a 
given  direction  by  motioning  with  the  hand;  but  even  through  the 
seventh  month  this  could  be  done  but  imperfectly,  as  she  was  apt 
to  watch  the  hand  or  face,  instead  of  looking  on  in  the  direction 
indicated. 

The  177th  day  (near  the  end  of  the  sixth  month)  disturbed  a 
little  while  nursing,  she  sat  up  and  looked  around,  and  caught  sight 
of  a  knot  of  cords  on  an  ottoman  close  by,  and  reached  for  it.  Her 
mother  set  her  on  it.  She  looked  all  about  for  it,  leaning  this  way 
and  that  to  look,  and  when  taken  back  to  be  nursed  kept  stopping 
and  looking  for  it  at  intervals.  Her  surprise  at  its  disappearance 
and  confusion  as  to  its  direction  were  very  quaint.  Nearly  two 
weeks  before  I  had  observed  that  she  would  remember,  even  after 
nursing  some  moments,  to  look  back  to  where  she  had  noticed  a 
bit  of  paper  or  the  like  on  the  floor. 

By  the  end  of  the  sixth  month,  she  would  usually  turn  and  look 
very  intelligently  into  the  face  of  anyone  calling  her. 

In  the  ninth  month  she  became  able  to  follow  a  pointing  finger 
easily  and  correctly.  Early  in  the  tenth  (281st  day)  she  did  it  so 
well  as  instantly  to  locate  a  small  black  kitten's  head  thrust  from 
a  wood  pile  some  fifteen  or  twenty  feet  away. 

By  the  middle  of  the  second  year  she  seemed  to  have  a  very 
clear  sense  of  direction,  judging  from  the  precision  with  which  she 
could  go  to  any  desired  spot  about  the  house  and  garden  ;  but  I 
suspect  this  was  largely  a  memory  of  objects  that  served  as  guides. 
In  the  103d  week  she  could  not  find  the  lounge  in  the  dark,  in  a 
room  perfectly  familiar. 

The  623d  day  she  was  quite  puzzled  by  a  difficulty  of  direction. 
She  had  discovered  the  trick  of  looking  between  her  legs,  and, 
wishing  to  look  at  her  mother  in  this  manner,  turned  her  face 
toward  her  and  stooped.  Surprised  at  seeing  objects  in  the  oppo- 
site direction,  she  tried  it  over  again,  with  the  same  result,  and  her 
mother  then  helped  her  out. 


shinn.-  The  Development  of  a  Child.  25 

Color. 

On  the  twenty-third  day  I  tried  in  vain  to  get  any  attention 
from  the  baby  to  a  red  silk  kerchief,  brilliantly  lighted  by  the  sun. 
But  on  the  25th  day,  in  the  evening,  having  caught  sight  of  my 
dark  red  gown,  barely  within  her  field  of  vision,  in  strong  lamp- 
light, she  threw  her  head  far  back  to  see  it,  with  an  expression  of 
interest,  such  as  she  had  never  had  before.  Yet  on  the  37th 
day  she  stared  as  earnestly  at  a  dark  blue  sack  I  wore,  where  the 
high  light  from  a  window  struck  my  shoulder.  Neither  of  these 
garments  was  of  glossy  material,  both  soft  wool;  yet  I  thought  it 
probable  that  the  high  light  and  not  the  color  was  in  both  cases  the 
attraction.  After  this  her  eyes  dwelt  a  good  deal,  for  a  few  days, 
on  the  red  gown,  even  when  no  direct  light  was  on  it,  but  there  was 
no  demonstration  of  interest.  Later  in  the  same  week,  the  sixth, 
on  the  42d  day,  her  eyes  followed  persistently  a  bunch  of  yellow 
"chrysanthemums,  and  returned  constantly  to  dwell  on  another 
bunch  pinned  on  my  breast.  I  procured  a  bunch  of  bright  red  gera- 
niums of  similar  size  and  form,  which  was  followed  when  I  persist- 
ently attracted  her  attention,  but  less  readily;  a  bunch  of  pink  gera- 
niums scarcely  at  all;  but  fatigue  counted  for  something  in  this, 
and  when  I  returned  to  the  chrysanthemums,  she  was  not  as  intent 
as  at  first. 

At  the  beginning  of  the  ninth  week,  57th  day,  she  once  looked 
fixedly  at  my  red  ribbon,  and  her  mother  told  me  that  the  day 
before  the  baby  had  caught  sight  of  a  red  bow  at  her  neck,  stopped 
nursing  to  stare  at  it,  and  would  not  go  on  till  it  was  put  out  of 
sight. 

The  first  day  of  the  third  month,  62d  day,  I  tried  to  find  her 
preference  by  suspending  two  long  strips,  one  red  and  one  yellow, 
before  her,  at  equal  distance  to  right  and  left,  so  that  she  would 
have  to  turn  her  head  slightly  to  look  at  either.  She  gazed  at 
first  one  and  then  the  other  with  some  interest,  then  neglected  both 
for  the  bright  button  heads  in  the  canopy  to  which  they  were  fas- 
tened. The  next  day  I  found  her,  after  a  long  sleep  and  in  high 
good  humor,  making  demonstrations  of  pleasure  over  them,  with 
arms  moving,  smiles,  and  murmurs,  but  unfortunately,  someone 
had  knotted  them  together,  so  she  saw  both  at  once.     On  the  68th 


26  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

day,  also,  I  was  told  of  her  making  joyous  demonstrations  over  « 
bunch  of  bright  sachets  hung    from  the  canopy  (not  in   motion 
These  were  her  first  signs  of  joy  in  color;  but  she  was  not  in  an^ 
respect  as  demonstrative  at  this  stage  as  Preyer's  child. 

During  the  fourth  month  I  noted  several  instances  of  attentive 
gazing  at  bright-colored  objects  —  a  pink  and  white  fan,  yellow  daf- 
fodils, a  red  shawl,  e.  g. ;  the  120th  day  she  stared  with  raised  brows 
and  look  of  surprise  at  the  flowers,  which  lay  on  the  table,  and 
the  shawl,  which  lay  on  a  chair,  as  if  she  recognized  the  color  as 
unfamiliar  in  those  places.  From  this  time  to  about  the  middle 
of  the  sixth  month,  the  daffodils  were  about  the  house  in  great 
quantities,  and  possibly  helped  to  develop  her  perception  of  yellow. 
On  the  133d  day  —  early  in  the  fifth  month  —  I  was  told  she  stopped 
crying  to  look  at  a  jar  of  them,  and  from  that  time  I  note  her  inva- 
riable interest  in  them;  when  she  came  to  seize,  her  arms  were 
always  stretched  out  for  them,  and  the  first  time  she  cried  with  the 
least  persistence  at  denial  was  for  them;  ajar  of  them  in  the  su\. 
called  out  excited  movements.  She  had  never  shown  so  uniforn. 
interest  in  any  object.  But  with  this  exception,  I  have  no  notes  of 
consequence  of  color  interest  during  the  fifth  month  till  near  the 
end;  a  few  colored  objects  were  noticed,  but  no  more  than  uncol- 
ored  ones.  (It  is  worth  observing,  however,  that  up  to  the  end  of 
the  third  month  colored  objects  attracted  far  more  attention  than 
uncolored.)  Except  for  the  daffodils,  hard,  bright  objects  were 
preferred  to  soft,  colored  ones  to  play  with;  a  silver-nickel  call-bell 
especially,  a  small  steel  bell,  napkin  rings,  etc.  Near  the  end  of  the 
month,  146th  day,  she  first  tried  to  pull  off  my  neck  ribbon,  a  yel- 
low one;  she  had  not  noticed  the  red  and  pink  ones  I  had  been 
wearing,  and  next  day  did  not  notice  a  red  one.  Yet  the  141st 
day  she  took  but  little  interest  in  a  yellow  Indian  basket,  about  the 
size  and  color  of  a  jar  of  daffodils  that  usually  stood  in  the  same 
place;  and  on  the  same  day  showed  no  interest  in  brown  and  yel- 
low figures  on  the  piano  cover,  but  tried  to  seize  red  ones  on  the 
table  cover,  while  on  the  147th,  she  reached  arms  for  ajar  of  pink 
and  purple  hyacinths  and  one  of  daffodils  with  equal  interest. 

Throughout  the  sixth  month  one  of  the  most  interesting  objects 
to  her  was  a  colored  picture  of  daffodils,  which  hung  low  on  the 
wall.     I  first  note  her  reaching  for  this,  among  other  objects,  on  the 


Shinn.  ]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  27 

second  day  of  the  month  (153d);  and  from  this  time  to  the  last 
week  of  the  month  my  notes  record  an  invariable  interest  in  it; 
she  would  be  diverted  by  it  even  when  crying.  No  picture  at  this 
time  was  recognized  as  a  representation,  and  her  liking  must  have 
been  due  either  to  the  color,  or  to  the  fact  that  it  was  the  only 
bright  picture  low  enough  on  the  wall  for  her  to  touch  and  look 
closely  at.  On  the  day  that  she  first  noticed  it,  she  reached 
also  for  the  yellow  Indian  basket;  but  so  too  for  any  flowers,  and 
for  objects  not  colored, —  curtain  cords,  books,  newspapers.  On 
the  154th  she  reached  for  a  colored  linen  book  among  all  her  toys, 
and  spent  some  time  dabbing  at  the  pictures  and  trying  to  pick 
them  up;  and  again,  cared  for  none  of  her  toys  till  a  red  and  yellow 
celluloid  ball  was  held  to  the  light,  —  this  she  reached  for  with 
apparent  excitement;  three  days  later  I  note  again  her  interest  in 
the  ball,  but  after  this  she  ceased  to  care  for  it  much. 

A  few  other  colored  objects  were  among  those  that  she  wished 
to  have  or  liked  to  play  with  in  the  sixth  month,  especially  a 
little  bright  blue  bottle.  We  thought  her  more  certain  to  grasp  at 
our  ribbons,  and  with  more  desire,  if  they  were  yellow,  and  once, 
when  denied  a  yellow  one,  she  remembered  it  with  a  whine  for  some 
seconds,  but  she  would  snatch  even  at  white  ones,  and  she  plucked 
at  my  red  gown  (149th  day).  The  181st  day,  just  before  the  end 
of  the  sixth  month,  I  tried  to  test  her  by  dangling  ribbons  of  various 
colors  before  her;  but  she  grasped  always  the  one  that  received 
the  strongest  light  from  the  window ;  and  when  we  put  them  in 
equal  light,  turned  from  one  to  another  with  equal  joy. 

From  the  beginning  of  the  sixth  month  to  the  end  of  the  year, 
flowers  were  perhaps  her  favorite  playthings.  I  took  much  note  of 
her  color  preferences  among  them.  The  following  were  the  most 
marked  indications :  — 

Sixth  month,  156th  day,  regarded  a  patch  of  orange-colored 
marigolds  (to  which  her  attention  was  drawn)  very  earnestly,  with 
motions  of  her  hands  toward  them.  When  wheeled  close  to  a 
hedge  of  Japanese  quince,  all  cherry  red,  showed  more  excitement, 
leaning  out  with  outstretched  arms  and  babbling  to  it.  This  hedge 
continued  to  excite  her  as  long  as  it  remained  in  flower, —  she  never 
passed  it  without  leaning  out  and  reaching  for  it,  or  babbling  to  it. 
It  made  a  very  large  expanse  of  color,  however,  larger  than  any 


28  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

other  she  saw,  which  may  have  had  as  much  to  do  with  her  inter- 
est as  the  fact  that  it  was  red. 

173d  day,  showed  unusual  excitement  and  desire  over  some 
yellow  buttercups  in  her  mother's  hat;  would  not  be  pacified  with- 
out them,  and  when  her  mother  had  left  the  room  to  put  them  out 
of  sight,  the  baby  looked  for  them  on  her  reappearance,  and  ex- 
pressed discontent  on  seeing  them  gone. 

Seventh  month,  2026  day,  cries  of  pleasure  at  a  clump  of  yellow 
oxalis  (to  which,  however,  her  attention  was  called). 

Ninth  month,  late,  decided  preference  appeared  for  the  orange- 
colored  marigolds. 

Tenth,  preference  for  marigolds  lasts  through  the  month, —  she 
reaches  for  them  past  scarlet  geraniums.  Toward  the  end  of  the 
month,  however,  she  would  beg  for  other  orange-colored  flowers, 
for  golden-rod,  and  for  red  flowers,  also  for  oleanders  and  pink  roses, 
even  pale  pink.  White  roses  she  became  very  desirous  of  about  the 
middle  of  the  month;  but  their  contrast  with  the  dark  green  leaves 
made  them  quite  conspicuous  on  the  vine.  293d  day  for  the  first 
time  she  showed  pleasure  and  desire  over  a  blue  flower,  a  large  and 
showy  African  lily;  but  never  again  in  the  first  year. 

Eleventh  month,  pink  roses  seemed  at  first  her  favorites;  but 
after  the  middle  of  the  month,  her  preferences  ceased  to  be  fixed, — 
she  would  want  now  pink  and  red  flowers,  rejecting  large  yellow 
primroses,  and  later  the  same  day  put  everything  aside  for  the 
marigolds  again.  Of  course  other  things  than  color  enter  into  these 
choices,  chiefly  convenience  of  form,  texture,  and  taste  for  han- 
dling and  mouthing;  the  advantage  in  these  respects  was  with  the 
roses.  321st  day  noticed  "heliotrope-colored"  flowers  on  a  gown, 
and  thereafter  was  interested  in  that  gown  and  its  flowers. 

Other  color  indications  in  the  second  six  months :  — 

Seventh  month,  201st  day,  did  not  notice  the  substitution  of  a 
white  rubber  nipple  for  a  black  one,  on  which  she  was  very  depend- 
ent in  going  to  sleep. 

Ninth  month,  last  week,  did  not  notice  especially  gilt  braid  and 
ornaments  on  a  yachting  gown,  nor  brass  fittings  on  the  yacht. 

Tenth  month,  269th  day,  tested  her  again  with  ribbons  of  bright, 
clear  tones,  and  the  results  seemed  to  show  preference  for  yellow 


Shinn.)  The  Development  of  a  Child.  29 

and  orange,  next  red;  blue  and  pink  were  entirely  neglected.1  I  had 
no  green  ribbon. 

In  the  middle  of  the  eleventh  month  I  tried  again;  preference 
for  orange  and  scarlet,  but  not  strong  preference.  Of  five  little 
books  she  was  fond  of  playing  with,  which  were  dull  shades  of 
cream,  yellow,  brown,  and  green,  she  preferred  the  green;  green 
leaves,  however,  never  seemed  to  interest  her.  She  was  habitually 
interested  in  large  yellow  butterflies  at  this  time.  On  the  321st 
day,  I  was  told,  she  refused  to  have  on  her  white  sunbonnet,  insist- 
ing on  her  pink  one. 

Late  in  the  twelfth  month,  3 5 2d  day,  she  distinctly  preferred 
two  bright  blue  books  to  a  bright  red  and  a  bright  yellow  one  on 
the  same  shelf. 

My  general  impression  of  her  color  liking  this  year,  both  at  the 
time  and  afterward  in  analyzing  my  notes,  was  that  it  did  not  play 
a  large  part  in  her  interests.  The  small  number  of  notes  of  color 
interest,  considering  how  closely  I  gleaned,  is  noticeable.  It  was 
also  unaccountably  variable;  a  surface  of  bright  color  would  occa- 
sionally bring  out  signs  of  great  pleasure,  and  at  other  times  was 
passed  with  indifference,  when  no  condition  was  perceptibly  altered. 
But  this  variability  was  more  or  less  characteristic  of  all  her  inter- 
ests. 

It  was  also  the  impression  of  those  who  watched  her,  even  more 
decidedly  than  the  notes  indicate,  that  yellow  attracted  her  most, 
then  orange  and  red,  and  pink,  while  blue  and  violet  were  scarcely 
noticed,  green  still  less.  But  we  were  also  satisfied  that  a  bright 
surface  of  the  cold  colors  was  preferred  to  a  dull  one  of  the  warm 
colors. 

During  the  second  year,  the  child's  color  sense  was  mainly  ob- 
servable in  connection  with  the  learning  of  color  names. 

In  the  first  part  of  the  thirteenth  month  it  was  a  favorite  occu- 
pation to  turn  over  the  leaves  of  a  picture  book,  pointing  to  sepa- 
rate objects  in  the  pictures,  and  asking  with  an  interrogative  sound 
to  have  them  named;  in  this  way  it  chanced  that  she  was  repeat- 

'Mrs.  Sharpe  first  tried  her  boy  with  colors  in  the  seventh  month,  219th 
day.  He  would  drop  red,  blue,  or  green  ribbons  to  grasp  either  scarlet  or  gray, 
but  reached  for  the  scarlet  in  prelerence  to  the  gray  every  time. 


30  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

edly  told,  "That  is  the  white  kitty,"  "the  drown  kitty."  When 
asked,  however,  "  Where  is  the  white  kitty?"  she  could  not  tell, 
though  in  the  habit  of  pointing  out  objects  in  the  pictures  when 
asked.  The  word  white  evidently  added  nothing  to  the  idea  of 
kitty.  By  the  end  of  the  month  she  did  know  which  was  the  "white 
kitty"  in  the  particular  picture,  but  not  which  kittens  were  white  in 
other  pictures;  she  had  simply  attached  "white-kitty"  as  a  name  to 
that  one,  easily  recognized  by  position.  Meantime,  on  three  sepa- 
rate days  in  the  fifty-fifth  week,  after  having  had  red flower  and  white 
flower,  red  string  and  white  string,  carefully  named  to  her,  {string 
and  flmver,  like  kitty,  being  already  well  understood,)  she  several 
times  selected  the  color  called  for,  though  she  also  failed  several 
times  in  the  case  of  the  "strings"  (bits  of  zephyr).  My  impression 
was  hardly  that  she  fixed  the  difference  in  name  by  the  difference 
in  color,  but  rather  that  she  managed  to  keep  distinct  for  a  few 
seconds,  in  spite  of  changes  of  position,  the  objects  each  as  a  whole. 

On  the  last  day  of  the  month,  the  396th,  I  namecj.  over  to  her 
the  colors  of  my  books  as  they  stood  on  the  shelves;  this  interested 
her  very  much,  and  she  urged  me  to  continue,  pointing  with  an 
asking  sound.  In  the  fourteenth  month  likewise,  when  someone 
named  to  her  the  colors  of  the  roses  in  a  panel  picture,  she  was 
pleased,  and  would  afterward  (fifty-eighth  week)  ask,  pointing  to  one 
rose  after  another,  to  be  told,  "That  is  a  pink  rose;  a  yellow  rose; 
a  red  rose;  a  white  rose."  I  could  not  see,  however,  throughout  the 
month,  that  she  remembered  these  names  at  all,  or  knew  what  they 
meant.  I  tried  her  several  times,  giving  her  always  the  color  ad- 
jective in  connection  with  the  perfectly  familiar  nouns,  book,  rose, 
ribbon;  but  if  the  distinction  was  caught,  it  was  with  difficulty, 
and  for  but  a  few  seconds,  —  a  marked  contrast  to  the  ease  with 
which  she  picked  up  and  held  the  names  of  objects.  Yet  on  the 
416th  day  she  put  her  finger  on  an  ink  spot,  saying,  "Dark!"  —  a 
word  hitherto  used  of  a  dark  room  only;  showing  some  ability  to 
abstract  the  idea  of  a  quality,  a  color  quality  at  that,  from  one  ob- 
ject and  apply  it  to  another. 

Toward  the  end  of  the  fifteenth  month  I  made  a  good  many  ef- 
forts to  have  her  distinguish  between  the  black,  white,  red,  and  blue 
stripes  of  an  afghan,  but  in  vain.  Beyond  these  few  experiments, 
and  the  incidental  use  of  color  words,  like  others,  in  talking  with  her, 
nothing  was  done  to  hasten  her  use  of  them. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  31 

Yet  near  the  end  of  the  sixteenth  month  she  was  suddenly 
found  in  possession  of  "  red."  On  the  481st  day  I  was  told  that  she 
had  pointed  to  a  red  book,  saying,  "We!  we!"  and  was  not  satis- 
fied till  her  mother  asked,  "  Does  baby  mean  red?"  The  next 
day,  as  she  played  by  me  on  a  red  mahogany  sofa,  she  put  her  hand 
on  the  wood,  saying,  "We!"  persistently;  at  last  I  asked,  "Does 
Ruth  mean  red?"  and  she  assented.  "Show  aunty  more  red,"  I 
said,  and  after  some  hesitation,  as  the  request  was  urged,  she  pointed 
out  the  red  carpet,  then  the  red  table  cover,  without  any  help. 

Later  in  the  same  day  I  held  her  before  a  colored  picture  of 
flowers  on  the  wall,  and  asked  her,  "Where  is  some  red?"  and  she 
put  her  finger  on  some  clear  red  nasturtiums.  Again,  after  a  con- 
siderable interval,  I  showed  her  red,  yellow,  and  white  ribbons,  and 
told  her  to  take  the  red ;  she  did  so  two  or  three  times,  after  slight 
hesitation.  Then  I  placed  a  number  of  books,  bright  red,  yellow, 
blue,  and  green,  before  her,  and  told  her  to  find  the  red,  which  she 
did  several  times.  I  then  took  her  to  the  picture  of  roses,  but  here 
she  was  confused  between  red  and  pink,  and  then  once  even  pointed 
to  white. 

It  was  strange  that  the  mahogany,  which,  though  quite  a  pure 
red,  was  so  dark  that  even  persons  fairly  versed  in  colors  might  have 
called  it  brown,  should  have  been  almost  the  first  thing  to  which 
she  applied  the  word;  we  had  certainly  never  named  any  such 
shade  to  her  as  red. 

This  incident  proved  one  of  the  instances  that  I  often  noticed  of 
a  sort  of  anticipation  of  a  power  not  really  possessed  till  later.  Two 
days  after  she  was  not  able  to  point  out  the  red  book  on  a  shelf, 
and  seized  yellow,  green,  and  brown  with  confidence  instead.  I 
then  dropped  the  experiment,  and  she  showed  no  farther  thought 
of  naming  colors  for  more  than  a  month. 

On  the  last  day  of  the  seventeenth  month  (517th)  hearing  me 
say  something  of  a  red  pencil,  she  began  looking  at  two  she  held  in 
her  hand,  first  one  and  then  the  other,  asking,  "Red?  red?" — 
"Which  is  the  red  one?"  I  asked.  She  offered  the  black  one  in- 
quiringly, and  when  I  said,  "That  is  n't  red,"  withdrew  it  and  offered 
the  other,  which  was  red,  with  confidence,  crying,  'Red."  I  now 
told  her  to  point  out  the  red  in  my  gown ;  at  first  she  dabbed  at  it 
anywhere,  saying,  "  Red,"  then  more  carefully  pointed  out  the  red 
stripe.     A  disposition  instantly  to  answer  any  question,  right   or 


32  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

wrong,  doubtless  caused  many  of  her  errors,  first  and  last.     Soon 
after,  she  pulled  a  red  book  from  a  shelf,  crying,  "Red!" 

A  week  later,  in  the  eighteenth  month,  I  asked  her,  as  she  was 
playing  with  some  books,  "  What  color?"  She  said,  "  Red "  (We) 
then  "Le,"  without  fixing  on  a  book.  With  some  trouble,  we  sat- 
isfied ourselves  that  "18"  meant  yellow.  She  then  added,  "Boo," 
pointing  first  hesitantly  to  a  red  book,  then  positively  to  a  blue  one. 
She  seemed  so  clear  and  triumphant  about  the  blue,  and  had  so 
evidently  picked  up  two  new  color  names  without  any  purposed 
teaching,  that  I  brought  out  a  set  of  Prang's  color  tablets  and  let 
her  take  them.  Told  to  show  red,  yellow,  and  blue,  she  did  so 
correctly  often  enough  to  be  quite  striking;  but  unfortunately  I 
kept  no  account,  as  it  happened  suddenly,  and  I  was  unwilling  to 
lose  her  streak  of  interest  by  going  for  paper  and  pencil. 

Once,  being  told  to  show  the  blue,  she  chose  out  one  after 
another  all  the  blue  and  violet  tablets  (four,  I  think,  including  light, 
dark,  and  green  blues),  and  held  up  one  after  another,  saying,  "  Boo," 
each  time;  afterward  she  gathered  them  together  in  her  own  hand 
and  looked  at  them  with  interest.  It  is  to  be  noticed  that  there 
were  green  tablets  in  the  pile,  but  she  seemed  to  experience  not  the 
least  difficulty  in  separating  the  blue  from  them.  Her  interest  in 
the  newly  named  color  overshadowed  that  in  red  and  yellow  (aided 
doubtless  by  our  pleasure),  and  she  was  disposed  to  say,  "  Boo," 
hastily  for  any  color. 

Besides  the  discovery  of  blue,  a  notable  step  was  that  she  un- 
derstood the  question,  "What  color?"  —  at  all  events  knew  when  we 
said  "color"  wliat  trait  of  the  object  we  wished  her  to  name. 
Hitherto  we  had  used  the  converse  question,  "Show  me  the  red," 
and  had  heard  it  named  by  her  only  when  she  volunteered  it. 

Three  days  later  I  tried  her  again  with  the  tablets  (I  had  tried 
her  once  in  the  interval,  but  have  only  note  of  "fairly  correct  an- 
swers"); and  as  I  held  up  red,  blue,  and  yellow,  she  called  each  cor- 
rectly; then,  getting  a  little  excited,  gave  wild  answers,  confusing 
red  and  blue,  —  the  beginning,  as  it  proved,  of  a  thorough  confusion. 

I  did  not  try  the  tablets  again  within  the  eighteenth  month;  but 
on  the  536th  day,  reaching  for  books,  she  asked  for  a  blue  one  as 
"red."     Told  that  it  was  not  red,  she  tried  "yellow,"  then  "blue;" 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  33 

then,  apparently  to  be  sure  of  being  right  this  time,  called  one  blue 
that  was  in  fact  red.  A  couple  of  hours  afterward  she  pointed  to  a 
blue  umbrella,  calling  it  red;  and  when  told  that  it  was  blue,  turned 
to  my  red  neck  ribbon  and  asserted  that  it  was  blue,  as  also  the  red 
lacing  cord  of  my  blouse;  both  were  bright  red,  nearly  pure. 

From  the  end  of  the  fourteenth  month  she  had  used  the  word 
black,  confusing  it  more  or  less  with  dirty,  perhaps  because  she  often 
saw  hands  soiled  with  taking  up  coal,  or  soiled  her  own  on  the  coal 
hod ;  she  had  first  picked  up  the  word  in  hearing  it  said  of  ink  spots. 
She  now,  in  the  eighteenth  month,  nearly  dropped  its  use  to  mean 
dirty,  and  in  the  seventy-sixth  week  would  point  out  the  black 
stripes  on  the  afghan,  saying,  "Ba!"  533d  day,  asked,  "What  color 
are  your  stockings?"  would  answer,  "  Black."  The  recognition  of 
blackness  (including  any  dark  gray,  soiled  aspect)  as  a  quality  was 
really  earlier  and  more  spontaneous  than  that  of  color  proper. 

As  to  her  color  preferences  during  the  six  months,  red  seemed 
at  first  favored.  In  the  thirteenth  month,  375th  day,  she  discovered 
and  watched  with  interest  a  dull  red  sunset;  377th,  took  marked  in- 
terest in  my  yellow  ribbon,  and  chose  her  buff  sunbonnet  rather 
than  her  pink;  but  382d,  and  for  some  days,  was  very  desirous  of 
an  aunt's  scarlet  knitting.  In  the  fourteenth  month,  416th  day, 
playing  on  a  pile  of  fresh  cobs,  she  carefully  picked  out  the  red 
ones;  and  on  the  426th  day  she  distinctly  preferred  red  books  on 
my  shelves.  Again,  in  the  fifteenth  month,  444th  day,  she  chose 
out  the  red  books;  on  the  450th,  seeing  her  pink  dress  under  a  pile 
of  others,  white  and  dark  blue,  she  drew  it  out  and  asked  by  mo- 
tions to  have  it  put  on,  and  clung  to  it  for  some  time. 

The  next  quarter,  however,  outside  of  the  interest  in  naming  red 
and  blue,  yellow  seemed  the  favorite.  During  the  seventeenth 
month  she  was  eagerly  interested  in  daffodils,  as  she  had  been  the 
year  before;  in  the  eighteenth,  yellow  oxalis  was  the  flower  con- 
stantly sought,  though  in  this  case  I  thought  the  long,  flexible 
stems,  which  she  enjoyed  pulling  and  handling,  the  chief  attraction. 
Orange-colored  eschscholtzias  and  yellow  mustards  were  next  favor- 
ites after  the  oxalis;  but  she  did  not  care  for  marigolds,  nearly  the 
same  color  as  the  poppies.  At  times,  for  some  days  in  both  seven- 
teenth and  eighteenth  months,  her  chief  desire  outdoors  was  to  run 


34  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

to  a  mallow  bush  and  pick  off  the  white  and  scarlet  seed  vessels; 
she  did  not  care  much  for  them  when  picked,  and  when  her  hands 
were  full  would  drop  them  and  seek  more.  Roses  always  pleased 
her,  and  in  the  latter  part  of  the  month,  lilacs;  once  I  saw  her  inter- 
ested in  some  red-purple  ixias  and  red  and  yellow  dwarf  fuchsias. 
The  first  time  a  blue  flower  was  noticed  in  the  second  year  was  the 
5  i  Stli  day,  when  she  ran  up  to  a  periwinkle,  crying,  "Baby,  fowa!" 
(a  flower  for  baby);  she  did  not  care  to  pick  it,  however,  nor  pay 
any  farther  attention  to  periwinkles.  The  536th  day,  she  was  much 
pleased  with  a  blue-purple  grass-flower. 

Late  in  the  eighteenth  month  I  saw  several  instances  of  compar- 
ison or  association  through  color.  On  the  536th  day,  seeing  a 
large  piece  of  red  cloth,  she  cried  insistently,  "Laly!"  (lady);  the 
only  explanation  was  that  a  lady  in  a  red  dress  had  been  at  the 
house  a  month  before.  Next  day,  looking  at  a  flower  catalogue, 
she  called  a  narcissus  a  rose.  "But  this  is  a  yellow  flower,"  I  said 
idly,  not  expecting  her  to  understand ;  but  she  cried,  "  Da ! "  (Then  it 
is  a  daffodil).  Yet  the  daffodils  had  been  gone  for  two  weeks.  The 
length  of  the  memory  is  more  surprising  in  the  first  instance,  but 
the  second  is  more  curious  in  the  word's  being  the  medium  of  mem- 
ory; for  the  resemblance  of  the  narcissus  in  the  picture  to  the  daf- 
fodils was  not  close.  A  third  instance  was  on  the  544th  day,  when 
she  recognized  as  a  lemon  one  that  by  freak  was  shaped  like  an 
orange;  it  must  have  been  by  the  paler  color  that  she  knew  them 
apart. 

In  the  nineteenth  month  came  a  distinct  ability  to  name  col- 
ors, and  with  it  a  great  increase  in  her  interest  in  color;  she  often 
spoke  of  it,  and  tried  to  name  the  colors  she  saw.  For  example,  on 
the  5 5 2d  day  she  pulled  a  piece  of  blue  silk  out  of  a  box,  and  said 
hesitantly,  "Red,"  then  decidedly,  "Blue;"  then  pulled  out  a  red 
bit  and  named  it  correctly.  Later,  chancing  to  look  up  to  where  a 
red  flag  was  tucked  in  the  rafters  (she  was  in  the  garret),  she  cried, 
"  Red ! " 

Several  times  on  this  day  and  the  next,  pointing  to  white  roses, 
she  cried,  "Rose!  white!"  (wo-wo!  fwi,  or  fa!)  Questioned,  "What 
color  is  the  rose?"  she  became  confused;  but  when  showed  that  it 


Shinn.] 


Tlie  Development  of  a  Child. 


35 


was  like  her  mother's  apron,  she  soon  pointed  to  it  and  cried, 
"  White ! "  No  one  had  ever  taught  her  white  since  our  futile  efforts 
three  months  before  to  see  if  she  could  distinguish  the  "white  rose," 
or  "white  kitten,"  but  she  had  heard  it  used  in  talk. 

On  the  557th  day  she  set  herself  to  name  over  the  colors  of  my 
books  as  she  pulled  them  from  the  shelf  and  piled  them  on  the  bed 
— red,  white,  blue.  I  again  had  no  pencil  at  hand,  but  kept  rapid 
count  with  my  fingers,  and  she  did  not  make  more  than  four  mis- 
takes out  of  fifteen  names.  She  even  pointed  out  on  parti-colored 
books  the  red  and  white.  This  voluntary  exercise  with  the  books 
was  repeated  from  time  to  time  for  months. 

The  progress  of  my  niece's  color  knowledge  thus  far  differed  so 
strikingly  from  that  of  Preyer's  child,  and  she  had  so  evidently 
passed  the  stage  at  which  regular  tests  were  begun  with  him,  that  I 
determined  to  apply  these,  and  an  hour  or  so  later  tried  her  with 
three  tablets.  The  results  were  not  nearly  as  good  as  in  her  spon- 
taneous exercises;  but  as  the  correct  answers  were  almost  always 
given  with  attention  and  decision,  the  incorrect  ones  either  inquir- 
ing!}' or  hastily  and  carelessly,  the  figures  understate  her  real  recog- 
nition. 

To  the  question,  "Where  is  the  red?  the  blue?  the  yellow?"  she 
answered :  — 


- 

Eight. 

Wrong. 

Red 

2 
2 

3 

Blue 

3 

To  the  question,  "What  color  is  this?' 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red 

6 
0 
0 

Yellow 

Blue  

3 
4 

Two  days  later,  naming  the  colors  of  books,  and  choosing  white 


36 


I  diversity  of  California. 


[Vol. 


and  red  especially,  she  made  no  mistakes.     Yet  with  the  tablets,  to 
the  question,  "Where  is  the  red?"  etc.:  — 


- 

Eight. 

Wrong. 

Red 

Would  not 

I 
4 

attend, 
o 

Blue 

0 

To  the  question,  "What  color?"  — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red 

Blue 

o 

2 
I 

3 

0 

2 

The  test  that  required  her  to  name  the  color  thus  far  proved  the 
more  difficult  one,  yet  when  she  named  colors  spontaneously  the 
demand  on  her  memory  of  words  was  the  same. 

These  two  tests  were  during  the  eightieth  week.  I  purposed 
hereafter  to  give  her  a  test  once  a  week.  But  after  the  eighty-first 
week  she  began  to  coax  for  them,  and  so  had  them  oftener.  The 
first  time,  on  her  mother's  suggestion  of  "the  colors,"  she  came  to 
me  coaxing,  "  Red!"  Yet  red  was  at  the  time  least  cared  for,  and 
she  often  refused  to  answer  regarding  it.  The  second  time  she  came 
without  suggestion,  and  begged,  "Blue-green!  blue-green  !  "  Next 
morning,  the  moment  I  entered  the  breakfast  room,  she  cried,  "  Red- 
green!  Upstairs!  Find!"  and  recurred  to  it  all  breakfast  time 
(574th  day).  Next  day  again,  hearing  something  said  of  "lips  getting 
blue,"  she  broke  in,  crying,  "Get  blue!  get  blue!"  and  had  to  be 
persuaded  to  finish  her  lunch.  Next  day  she  began  to  ask  for  them 
as  "red  green-too"  (2.  e.,  red  and  green);  581st  day,  suddenly,  in  the 
midst  of  quietly  eating  her  potato,  broke  out  with  a  demand  for 
"Red  green-too!"     By  the  end  of  the  twentieth  month  the  usual 


Shins.]  TllC  Development  of  a    CllUd.  37 

form  of  asking  was  for  "  red  and  green,"  sometimes  with  the  addi- 
tion of  " —  and  blue  too."  No  other  colors  were  ever  mentioned,  ex- 
cept that  once  in  the  twenty-first  month  (618th  day)  she  begged, 
"Ruth  have  pink  and  blue,"  over  and  over;  finally  "pink  and  blue 
—  and  green."  Yet  at  that  time  pink  had  never  been  included  among 
her  tablets.  During  the  twenty-first  month  she  was  becoming  more 
interested  in  their  forms  than  in  their  colors,  and  began  to  ask  for 
them  under  the  names  of  these.  On  the  637th  day:  "Ruth  see  red 
and  green?" — "Not  now,  dear;  Ruth  must  go  to  bed."  —  "Ruth  see 
red  and  green  by  and  by?  "  —  "  Yes."  —  "  And  blue?  "  —  "Yes."  — 
"And  triangle?" — -"Yes."  —  "And  round?"  The  next  day,  after 
she  had  treated  them  roughly,  and  they  had  been  put  away:  "  Have 
color?  Play  pretty!"  a  frequent  (though  rather  valueless)  promise 
under  such  circumstances,  notable  in  this  case  as  the  first  instance 
I  have  of  the  general  word  color,  —  "co." 

The  pleasure  taken  from  the  first  in  the  exercises  was  increased 
when,  at  the  end  of  the  eighty-second  week,  I  put  the  tablets  into  a 
little  box,  and  allowed  her  to  take  out  one  at  a  time,  naming  it. 
The  first  time  that  I  did  this  (575th  day),  she  thus  named  them  over 
and  over  for  twenty  minutes  without  fatigue.  During  the  whole 
time  the  tests  were  carried  on,  I  never  (except  on  two  or  three  oc- 
casions, when  I  was  showing  the  procedure  to  others)  continued 
them  after  her  interest  flagged.  After  the  close  of  the  nineteenth 
month  she  begged  daily  for  them,  often  several  times  a  day,  but 
sometimes  cared  little  for  them  when  she  got  them,  slapping  them 
about  roughly,  or  putting  them  into  her  mouth.  Yet  she  would 
whimper  and  beg  for  them  if  they  were  put  away.  If  they  were 
restored,  however,  after  she  had  once  showed  fatigue  thus,  it  never 
proved  possible  to  renew  any  real  interest  in  them.  After  the 
eighty-sixth  week  it  was  my  custom  to  let  her  have  them,  one  after 
another,  upon  naming  correctly;  she  thus  had,  after  every  lesson, 
the  privilege  of  playing  freely  with  them,  and  it  was  sometimes  half 
an  hour  before  she  tired  of  them.  She  was  also  occasionally  in  the 
twenty-first  month  given  the  whole  boxful  to  play  with  at  will,  ex- 
cept that  they  were  withdrawn  as  soon  as  she  began  to  play  roughly. 
I  watched  her  play  in  vain,  for  the  most  part,  for  significant  results 
as  to  color.     Such  results  as  did  appear  related  chiefly  to  form. 

In  the  eighty-first  week,  green  was  added.     She  was  told  the 


38 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


name  when  first  shown  the  tablet,  and  never  needed  to  be  told  again. 
The  result  of  four  tests,  eighty-first  and  eighty-second  week,  was:  — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Question. 

Red 

4 
4 
7 
9 

5 
o 
o 
c 

Blue 

"  Where  is  red  ?  "  etc. 

Red 

Blue 

i  (i) 
'9 
18(1) 

20 

i  r 
I 
o 
o 

"What  color  is  this?" 
and  volunteered 

answers. 

Red 

7 

27 

31 

37 

17 
i 
i 
I 

In  all.  ' 

Blue 

In  all  these  tests,  when  she  has  given  an  answer  wrong,  and  at 
once,  without  suggestion,  corrected  it,  I  have  noted  it  as  right,  but 
indicated  it  by  parentheses.  When,  on  the  other  hand,  she  has  cor- 
rected her  answer  after  being  asked  a  second  time,  I  have  set  it 
down  wrong,  though  it  was  often  heedlessly  made  in  the  first  place 
and  readily  corrected;  the  one  error  noted  above  as  to  yellow  was 
of  this  sort. 

It  is  to  be  noted  especially  that  not  the  least  confusion  between 
green  and  blue  appeared,  now  or  hereafter.  Once,  573d  day,  having 
pulled  down  my  books,  which  I  had  arranged  by  color  for  her  ben- 
efit, she  wished  to  put  them  back  herself,  and  put  a  green  book  be- 
tween blue  ones.  "O  no,"  I  said,  "put  the  green  book  with  the 
other  green  ones!"  and  altered  its  place.  After  that  she  three  or 
four  times,  carefully  and  after  consideration,  placed  green  and  blue 
books  in  the  proper  group. 

The  disposition  to  interchange  the  names  red  and  blue,  on  the 


1  One  test  is  included  here  in  which  I  failed  to  keep  separate  record  for  the 
two  forms  of  question,  and  the  figures  therefore  do  not  agree  as  totals  with 
tiiose  in  the  tables  above. 


Shinn]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  39 

other  hand,  increased;  and  practically  all  the  errors  in  red  and  blue 
recorded  in  the  whole  period  of  the  tests,  were  of  this  nature.  Blue 
was  not  so  much  affected  by  it  as  red,  especially  if  she  saw  it  before 
the  red.  On  the  other  hand,  I  noticed  in  the  eighty-third  week  that 
red  was  answered  right  without  hesitation  at  first;  then  as  soon  as 
blue  had  been  named  she  became  confused  between  them.  The 
confusion  seemed  to  occur  in  the  effort  to  remember;  when  she 
called  the  name  without  thought,  on  sight  of  the  color,  she  had  no 
trouble,  but  as  soon  as  she  began  to  think  about  it,  she  would 
remember  the  word  blue,  and  lose  confidence.  It  corroborates 
this  view  that  when  she  named  the  colors  of  objects  about  her,  which 
was  generally  done  on  impulse  without  the  least  thought,  she  was 
rarely  wrong.  The  563d  day,  e.g.,  she  was  greatly  interested  in  a 
red  jacket  I  wore,  crying, "  Red !  red ! "  as  soon  as  she  saw  it,  and  dur- 
ing the  day  from  time  to  time,  pointed  all  over  it,  sleeves,  breast,  col- 
lar, saying,  "  Red !"  So  red  and  blue  books,  ribbons,  handkerchiefs. 
Yet  she  did  sometimes  interchange  the  words  in  voluntary  comments 
on  color.  The  first  time  she  saw  my  books  arranged  by  color  she 
sought  the  blue  group  at  once,  calling  it  red;  the  590th  day  a  red 
ribbon  was  at  first  called  blue;  the  598th,  she  asked  if  the  red  trim- 
mings of  a  railway  car  were  blue;  and  the  612th  day  asked  me  ear- 
nestly and  repeatedly  for  a  "blue  pencil,"  not  even  perceiving  her 
mistake  when  I  looked  about  in  vain  for  such  a  thing  before  it 
occurred  to  me  that  she  meant  red. 

It  seems  to  me  unquestionable  that  the  difficulty  was  purely  a 
confusion  of  names.  Besides  the  evidence  of  incidents  already 
given,  the  following  could  hardly  have  been  possible  if  any  confu- 
sion of  vision  existed:  On  the  573d  day,  on  having  the  blue  tablet 
shown  her,  she  unhesitatingly  called  it  blue,  then  instantly  pointed 
to  a  bookcase  where  a  large  bright  blue  volume  was  conspicuous, 
and  cried,  "Blue! "  I  then  laid  a  red  book  before  her,  and  made  her 
understand  that  I  wished  her  to  lay  on  it  a  tablet  like  it ;  and  she 
did  so,  selecting  the  right  red,  though  she  had  two  different  red 
tablets. 

The  difficulty  in  naming  red  seemed  to  annoy  her,  and  give  her 
a  distaste  for  the  color  in  the  tests,  which  did  not  perceptibly  affect 
her  liking  for  it  under  other  circumstances.  In  the  eighty-first 
week,  asked,  "What  is  this?"  of  red,  she  would  give  no  answer, 


40 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


but  instead  point  to  and  name  the  yellow  or  green.  Once  when 
red  was  pressed,  she  decisively  picked  up  the  tablets  and  handed 
to  me,  saying,  "Way!"  (Put  away),  and  when  I  had  several  times 
ignored  this  request,  she  took  them  all,  carried  them  over  to  the 
mantel,  stood  on  tiptoe,  and  tried  to  put  them  up,  saying,  "Way!" 

The  discrepancies  in  the  number  of  answers  given  for  the  differ- 
ent colors  are  due  in  part  to  such  preferences  and  dislikes;  but  as 
there  were  two  red,  two  blue,  and  two  green  tablets  in  the  set  I 
used  at  this  time,  and  but  one  yellow,  the  degree  in  which  yellow 
was  sought  was  greater  up  to  the  eighty-sixth  week  than  the  fig- 
ures show.  The  light  and  dark  green,  light  and  dark  blue,  were 
recognized  with  equal  ease,  but  the  two  reds  were  not  so  easily 
seen  to  be  the  same  color,  and  about  the  end  of  the  month  I  laid 
aside  the  light  red  for  a  time. 

The  total  result  of  tests  for  the  three  weeks  of  the  nineteenth 
month  during  which  thev  were  carried  on,  was:  — 


- 

Eight. 

Wrong. 

Question. 

Red 

9 

IO 

iS 

15 

5 
2 

3 
0 

Blue 

Green 

"  Where  is,"  etc. 

Red 

IO 

20 

27 
28 

iS 

4 

10 

0 

Blue 

volunteer. 

Green 

Red 

Yellow  

Blue 

j4 
50 
5i 

1 

14 
1 

In  all.' 

Meantime  the  child  asked  almost  daily  about  the  colors  whose 
names  she  did  not  yet  know.  She  would  point  to  pink,  olive,  ecru, 
purple,  asking,  "Blue?"  "Red?"  "White?"—  not,  I  think,  that  she 
supposed  the  colors  were  these,  but  that  she  knew  no  other  way  to 
put  the  question,  as  she  had  not  yet  the  word  "color"  for  her  own 
use.     We  avoided  answering  concerning  mixed  or  doubtful  colors, 

1  See  note,  page  38. 


Shinn.  ] 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


41 


but  it  was  impossible  to  keep  her  from  widening  very  much  her 
knowledge  of  each  color  beyond  the  typical  ones  in  her  tablets. 
Once  on  the  574th  day,  perhaps  recalling  something  I  had  told  her 
some  days  before  of  a  baby  whose  hair  was  red,  she  put  her  hand 
on  my  hair,  and  suggested, "  Red?"  and  was  rather  disposed  to 
insist  till  I  told  her  it  was  brown,  which  she  accepted.  She  asked 
the  same  question  about  her  own  and  her  mother's,  which  are 
like  mine,  brown  with  no  red  tinge.  This  was  like  the  usual  for- 
mula for  asking  a  new  color  name,  but  I  was  unable  to  resist  the 
impression  in  this  case  that  she  was  struggling  with  some  chaotic 
association  or  inference:  "Hair.it  seems,  is  a  red  object;  this,  then, 
must  be  red,  in  spite  of  appearances." 

The  562d  day  I  kept  count  of  one  of  her  voluntary  exercises 
with  the  books,  and  found  that  she  named  them  right  (black,  white, 
green,  yellow,  red)  twenty-four  times  out  of  thirty. 

The  566th  day,  seeing  a  lamp  extinguished,  she  cried  eagerly, 
"White  lamp!  white  lamp!"  pointing  to  the  porcelain  shade,  from 
which  the  reddish  glow  was  now  gone.  Her  interest  in  this  change 
of  color  was  the  more  striking  as  the  lamp  had  been  put  out  by 
the  breaking  of  the  chimney,  amid  a  good  deal  of  commotion, 
which  she  regarded  with  entire  indifference.  So  repeatedly, 
"White  roses!"  White  seemed  especially  to  interest  her  at  this 
time  (eighty-first  week). 

Three  tests  in  the  eighty-third  week  gave: — 


- 

Right. 

Wrung. 

Question. 

Red 

5 
5 
6 

5 

2 
O 
O 

O 

Blue 

"  Where  is,"  etc. 

Red 

Blue 

3 
10 

14(2) 

I2(.) 

6 
0 

4 
0 

"What  color?  "  and 
volunteer. 

Red 

Yellow  

Blue 

8 

15 
22 
18 

8 
0 

4 
0 

In  all. 

42 


University  of  California. 


(Vol.  i. 


This  week  she  called  dark  green  or  dark  blue  books  black, 
especially  by  lamplight.  She  showed  some  odd  confusions  in  the 
very  colors  she  was  sure  of  in  the  tablet  tests.  The  577th  day 
she  called  a  white  book  yellow,  and  the  next  day  called  yellow  roses 
red,  and  told,  "O  no!"  tried  "blue."  The  563d  day  she  called  a 
white  book  green,  and  told,  "No,"  tried  ''yellow." 

She  now  began  to  try  herself  to  put  "off"  colors  under  the  cat- 
egories she  knew,  and  on  the  582d  day  called  an  olive  book  green, 
and  a  red-brown  one  brown.  The  ability  to  discern  the  real  color 
in  the  olive  shade  was  surprising;  and  I  was  unable  to  tell  how  she 
learned  to  name  the  brown,  for  though  she  knew  brown  sugar  and 
brown  bread  very  well,  and  had  had  brown  hair  named  to  her,  nei- 
ther of  these  browns  resembled  the  color  of  the  book  at  all  nearly. 
She  may  have  had  a  wide  general  idea  of  brown,  or  she  may  have 
been  told  this  particular  shade  among  her  questionings. 

Black  and  white  were  now  added  to  the  tablets.  Two  tests  in 
the  eighty-fourth  week  gave: — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Question. 

Red 

3(2) 

4 

8 

8 

3 

3 

I 

0 
0 
0 
0 
0 

Blue 

Green 

Black 

White 

"  Where  is  the,"  etc. 

Red 

4(8) 
11 

21(7) 
26 

6 

2 

0 
0 
I 

1 

0 
2 

Blue 

"  What  color?  "  anil 
volunteer. 

Black 

White 

And  the  total  result  of  three  in  the  eighty-fourth  and  eighty- 
fifth  weeks  was: — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red                        

19 
19 

45 

4 
0 

1 

Green 

43 
M 
10 

1 

Black 

White 

0 

Blue 

Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  43 

In  tlie  eighty-fourth  week  red  suddenly  recovered  from  the  con- 
fusion with  blue,  but  for  the  one  week  only.  It  is  evident  from  the 
results  of  that  week,  as  well  as  from  all  the  former  results  with 
colors  other  than  red  and  blue,  that  apart  from  the  special  difficulty 
in  naming  those  two  colors,  it  made  no  particular  difference  in 
which  form  the  question  was  put;  her  habit  of  voluntarily  attach- 
ing names  to  colors  about  her  made  it  as  easy  as  to  find  colors  to 
fit  the  names.  After  the  eighty-fourth  week,  therefore,  I  ceased  to 
keep  separate  record;  the  usual  form,  however,  was,  "  What  color?" 
and  in  very  many  cases  the  colors  were  spontaneously  named. 

The  one  case  in  which  I  ever  heard  blue  called  green  occurred 
in  the  eighty-fourth  week,  and  was  evidently  mere  heedlessness. 
White  in  the  same  week  was  twice  called  yellow.  In  the  eighty- 
fifth  week,  590th  day,  she  called  pink  roses  white;  I  thought  this 
might  be  due  to  ignorance  of  the  word  pink,  but  later  occurred  in- 
stances of  real  confusion  of  white  and  pink. 

She  had  fits  of  pointing  to  and  naming  all  the  colors  about  her. 
On  the  590th  day,  c.  g.,  she  asked  (correctly)  for  a  "red  book,"  and 
carried  it  about,  commenting,  "Red  book!  red  book!" — then 
pointed  to  the  walnut  banisters, observing,  "Brown!"  then  to  a  "red 
pail,"  then  ran  into  an  adjoining  room  and  showed  me  "blue  birdies" 
on  a  Japanese  wall  decoration. 

In  the  eighty-sixth  week,  orange  was  added.  She  had  not 
heard  this  color  name  used  incidentally,  as  she  had  black,  brown, 
etc.,  but  it  delighted  her  very  much,  and  never  had  to  be  named 
but  once.  It  was  given  to  her  at  the  same  time  (598th  day)  that 
she  received  a  present  of  a  large  number  of  new  tablets,  in  various 
geometrical  forms;  and  the  first  time  that  she  had  them  in  her 
hands,  she  sat  for  one  hour  in  her  mother's  lap  by  a  table,  happily 
occupied  in  gathering  out  over  and  over  all  the  orange  and  yellow 
tablets  (some  twenty  in  all),  although  she  was  tired,  and  kept  awake 
far  beyond  her  bedtime.  Two  days  later  they  were  put  into  her 
hands  again,  and  she  occupied  herself  again  in  the  same  way.  If 
told  to  pick  out  the  yellow  ones  she  did  not  care  to  do  so,  but  if 
left  unnoticed  gathered  up  every  orange  and  yellow  one,  over  and 
over,  with  absorbed  interest.  Yet  an  hour  later,  given  them  to  oc- 
cupy her  at  a  meeting,  she  did  not  care  much  for  them.  Until  her 
interest  was  freshly  stimulated  by  the  addition  of  orange,  she  had 
not  cared  as  much  for  the  tests  as  in  the  nineteenth  month. 


44 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


The  results  of  nine  tests  in  the  eighty-sixth  and  eighty-seventh 
weeks  follow.  One  of  these  was  made  before  a  roomful  of  people, 
after  bedtime  and  by  electric  light ;  yet  only  the  usual  error  as  ta 
red  and  blue  occurred.  Three  other  tests  were  made  in  the  eighty- 
sixth  week,  of  which  I  did  not  keep  record,  beyond  the  note  that 
the  only  error  was  this  usual  one,  which  she  always  corrected  as 
soon  as  her  attention  was  called  to  it.  With  the  beginning  of  the 
eighty- seventh  week  red  was  withdrawn  to  give  her  an  opportunity 
to  recover  from  what  was  becoming  a  fixed  habit  of  confusion;  so 
that  in  five  of  the  nine  tests  it  does  not  have  any  part. 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red 

96 

99 
104 

16 
I 
I 

2 

Black  

White 

46 

44 
Si 

0 
3 

Blue 

Green 

White  was  once  called  yellow  again.  The  610th  day  she  called 
a  piece  of  dingy  white  paper  yellow.  A  roll  of  terra  cotta  wall  pa- 
per was  called,  very  doubtfully,  orange.  It  was  in  fact  a  mixed  and 
imperfect  orange  shade;  but  few  grown  people  would  have  classified 
it  as  correctly. 

In  the  course  of  a  trip  made  on  the  597th  and  598th  days,  she 
was  alert  to  see  colors.  As  I  stood  by  with  her  in  my  arms  while 
her  mother  was  shopping,  she  began  pointing  out  the  stockings  on 
a  counter,  and  naming  over  the  colors,  —  'Green  stockings!  Red 
stockings!"  blue,  white,  pink,  black,  brown,  —  with  scarcely  a  slip- 
I  did  not  know  before  that  she  could  name  pink,  but  concluded 
that  she  had  picked  it  up  from  hearing  her  pink  dress  and  bonnet 
spoken  of.  Having  reason  to  think  she  knew  a  striped  dress,  I 
said,  "  Find  some  striped  stockings."  She  looked  all  about,  repeat- 
ing, "Find  striped  stockings!  No  striped  stockings!"  though  she 
was  looking  at  some;  after  some  seconds  it  came  to  her,  and  she 
pointed  to  them,  crying  joyously,  "Striped  stockings!"  and  then 
pointed  out  several  other  pair.  She  then  looked  about  the  store, 
and  pointed  out  near  and  far  "blue  shirt!"  "red  box!"  "green  box!" 
shouting  her  discoveries  aloud,  till  all  the  departments  looked  and 
smiled,   and    I    carried   her   close   to  the  boxes    to  moderate   her 


SlilNN.  ] 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


45 


tones;  she  then  named  over  quietly  to  me  the  colors  of  all  the 
boxes  in  a  row,  and  then  asked  for  "  more  box."  Soon  after,  taken 
into  a  strange  parlor,  her  first  act  was  to  run  to  a  small  statue  that 
stood  on  the  floor,  crying,  "  White  man !  "  and  embrace  it.  She  had 
never  seen  a  statue  before.  Some  hours  later,  after  a  railroad  trip, 
she  was  taken  to  a  strange  bedroom,  and  though  fretful  and  ab- 
sorbed in  desire  to  go  outdoors,  the  instant  she  entered  the  room, 
she  ran  across  it  to  look  at  and  name  the  silk  balls  hanging  from  the 
toilet  stand,  —  "blue,"  "yellow;"  then  was  puzzled  by  a  brownish 
violet,  and  asked  what  it  was.  In  the  library  she  went  as  usual  to 
the  book  shelves,  naming  the  colors  of  the  books,  though  she  would 
not  do  this  when  strangers  came  in.  Next  day  going  home  she 
asked  about  the  color  of  the  finishings  in  the  railway  car. 

Nearly  two  weeks  later  I  was  talking  to  her  of  this  trip,  and 
mentioned  our  taking  the  street  cars;  she  broke  in  with,  "Green 
car!"  Her  attention  had  in  fact  at  the  time  been  drawn  to  the  colors 
of  the  cars.  A  more  striking  instance  of  color  memory  occurred  on 
the  6ooth  day;  seeing  for  the  first  time  a  yellow  primrose  in  bloom 
bv  the  edge  of  the  daffodil  bed,  she  called  it  at  first  glance  a  daffodil ; 
yet  she  had  seen  no  daffodil  there  or  elsewhere  for  nearly  twelve 
weeks. 

The  6oist  day  I  tried  whether  she  could  hold  the  color  idea  in 
combination  with  another  demand,  as  the  variety  of  form  just  intro- 
duced into  her  tablets  seemed  to  have  caused  not  the  slightest  con- 
fusion; she  proved  able  to  select  a  "square  green,"  "oblong  yel- 
low," "round  black,"  tablet,  and  the  like,  with  visible  effort,  and 
with  some  helping  till  she  understood  what  was  wanted,  but  in  the 
main  correctly. 

Having  been  shown  the  difference  in  color  of  ripe  and  unripe 
loquats,  she  proved  able  to  use  it  quite  carefully  as  a  means  of  dis- 
crimination, selecting  the  yellow  and  rejecting  the  green. 
All  the  tests  in  the  twentieth  month  give: — 


- 

Right. 

\\  rang. 

- 

i;i  in. 

Wrong. 

Red 

25 
121 

'55 

154 

24 
j 

7 
4 

Black 

White 

60 

54 

Si 

0 
6 

Blue  

Orange 

46 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


Violet  was  introduced  in  the  middle  of  the  eighty-eighth  week. 
The  child  seemed  to  care  little  for  it,  and  several  times  refused  to 
name  it,  and  wished  the  colors  put  away  when  we  came  to  it.  She 
was  apt  to  call  it  blue,  but  distinguished  instantly  when  she  saw 
them  side  by  side.  Seen  in  the  shadow  it  was  once  or  twice  called 
black.  Shadow  did  not  confuse  her  as  to  yellow,  blue,  and  green, 
which  once  fell  behind  the  lounge,  and  were  promptly  named  by 
her  as  she  looked  down;  yet  once  in  the  ninetieth  week  a  yellow 
tablet  behind  the  lounge  was  called  orange  at  first  glance. 

Five  tests  in  the  eighty-eighth  and  eighty-ninth  weeks  gave: — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

46 
63 
55 
29 

0 
1 
0 
I 

White 

06 
31 

I 

Blue 

O 

Violet 

9 

Black  

I  made  one  effort  to  reintroduce  red,  but  the  old  mistake  was 
made,  and  I  put  it  away  again. 

The  624th  day,  playing  with  some  little  girls  (an  unusual  thing) 
and  in  a  new  place,  she  answered  questions  as  to  the  colors  of  flow- 
ers wildly,  calling  orange  ones  violet,  pink  ones  orange,  yet  she  an- 
swered other  questions  as  difficult  correctly. 

In  the  ninetieth  week,  pink  was  added.  Although  she  had  used 
this  name  of  her  own  notion  in  naming  objects,  she  could  not  name 
the  tablet  till  told,  and  afterward  asked  for  the  word  once  or  twice; 
she  never  misnamed  it,  however,  and  could  always  select  it.  She 
was  interested  in  it,  and  sought  the  pink  ones,  once  gathering  them 
all  out,  but  did  not  care  as  much  for  it  as  for  orange  on  its  first  in- 
troduction. 

Red  was  then  restored,  and  on  the  first  trial  but  one  mistake  was 
made;  on  the  second,  she  twice  began  to  call  it  blue,  then  instantly 
caught  herself  up  and  corrected  it.  Once  also  she  called  blue  red, 
hastily  corrected  to  blue,  then  pointed  to  red,  saying  emphatically, 
"  That  red."  Once  or  twice  she  picked  up  a  red  one  of  her  own 
accord,  saying,  "That  red!'  My  gown  she  instantly  recognized  as 
red. 

In  the  ninety-first  week,  brown  (burnt  umber)  was  added.     With 


Shinn.] 


The  Development  of  a  Cliild. 


47 


this,  as  with  the  pink,  in  spite  of  having  seemed  to  know  the  word, 
she  had  to  be  told  what  the  tablet  was;  and  two  days  after  had 
forgotten  the  name,  though  she  would  not  miscall  it,  and  answered 
readily  when  I  resorted  to  the  question,  "Where  is  the  brown?" 
She  did  not  care  much  for  it,  and  on  one  occasion  when  we  came 
to  it  lost  her  interest  in  the  lesson,  refused  to  try  to  name  it,  and 
began  turning  over  and  naming  those  already  earned  and  in  her  lap: 
"That  one  pink;  that  one  blue;"  etc. 

In   the  following  results  from  seven  tests,  in  the  ninetieth  and 
ninety-first  weeks,  red  enters  into  five,  brown  into  three. 


- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red  

25(2) 

30 

30(1) 

41 

3 
O 
0 
3 

Black 

13 
18 

28 

0 

3 
I 

Violet  .... 
Pink 

27 
28 

5 

White 
Orange.... 

Blue 

Brown  

In  one  of  these  tests,  633d  day,  after  once  or  twice  naming  white 
correctly,  she  began  persistently  calling  it  pink,  and  did  not  seem 
able  to  correct  herself;  this  did  not  seem  a  whim,  but  a  real  confu- 
sion. Even  when  the  question  was  changed  to,  "Where  is  the 
white?"  she  at  first  pointed  to  the  pink,  then  to  the  white,  then 
became  much  disinclined  to  go  on. 

The  one  mistake  made  in  orange,  —  the  only  one  ever  made,  — 
was  when,  after  she  had  gathered  up  all  the  pink  tablets,  she  looked 
for  more,  picked  up  an  orange  one,  and  said  that  it  was  pink.  This 
would  have  been  quite  natural  had  the  pink  used  been  salmon, 
which  is  an  orange  tint;  but  it  was  nearer  rose. 

The  630th  day  she  was  playing  with  some  samples  of  paper  of 
various  "off"  colors,  and  her  mother,  for  curiosity,  asked  her  to 
name  them.  She  not  only  named  difficult  greens  and  blues,  but 
called  lilac  "violet,"  —  this  several  times  consistently.  There  were 
two  tints,  one  very  light,  almost  lavender,  which  her  mother  said 
she  had  before  called  pink ;  a  week  later  she  called  a  gown  of  the 
red  violet  shade  called  heliotrope,  "violet."  A  vermilion  she  called 
orange;  salmon  also  she  called  orange.  Atone  between  green  and 
yellow  she  called  yellow;  we  regarded  it  as  green  (perhaps  wrongly) 


48 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


and  told  her,  "  No."  She  then,  without  suggestion,  found  the  yel- 
low tablet  and  compared  them,  then  said  the  paper  was  green. 

This  same  day,  on  a  street  corner,  I  told  her  we  must  wait  for  a 
blue  car.  She  recognized  the  right  car  as  soon  as  it  appeared, 
pointing  and  shouting,  "Blue  car!" 

In  the  twenty-first  month  (including  one  test  in  the  ninety-sec- 
ond week),  the  results  were:  — 


- 

Right. 

Wrong, 

Right. 

Wrong. 

- 

Right. 

Wrong. 

Red 

28 

82 

101 

i°3 

4 
0 

1 
3 

Black  .... 

46 
61 
S6 

I 

5 
1 

Violet 
Pink 
Brown 

60 
3° 

7 

Yellow 

Blue 

White 
Orange 

O 

Green 

In  the  twenty-second  month  she  was  away  from  home  for  a 
couple  of  weeks,  and  for  eighteen  days  had  no  trial  with  the  colors. 
I  visited  her  when  she  had  been  away  three  days,  and  her  first 
remark  after  greeting  was,  " Red  and  green?"  but  when  told  the 
colors  were  left  at  home,  thought  no  more  about  them.  On  her 
return,  after  having  been  at  home  some  hours,  interested  in  other 
recognitions,  she  suddenly  ran  to  the  closet,  crying  out  for 
"oblongs"  (657th  day).  The  forms  interested  her  for  about  three- 
quarters  of  an  hour;  but  finally  she  put  her  finger  on  an  orange 
tablet,  and  said  more  than  once,  "  That  one  red."  "Why,  no,  it  is 
orange,"  I  said.  She  looked  at  it  doubtfully,  and  finally  said, 
"Orange  color."  This  was  by  lamplight,  however;  and  when  I 
tried  next  morning  she  proved  to  know  the  whole  ten,  perhaps  not 
as  instantly  as  before  the  interval. 

She  was  not  so  easy  to  hold  to  the  lesson  now,  having  had 
the  tablets  to  play  with  freely  so  much,  and  perhaps  also  having 
more  will  of  her  own  as  she  grew  older;  on  this  day  she  was 
vexed  at  my  holding  them  away  from  her  till  she  had  named  them, 
and  said  repeatedly,  "Hold  down!  let  Ruth  have!"  Her  interest, 
too,  had  gone  away  to  the  forms.  It  was  indeed  unnecessary  to 
press  color  tests  any  farther,  and  I  dropped  them.  She  would 
sometimes,  however,  as  she  handled  the  tablets,  mention  their  colors. 
I  took  one  memorandum  of  such  an  occasion,  and  one  formal  test 


Shinn.  ] 


The  Development  of  a  Cliild. 


49 


after  her  return;  no  error  whatever  was  made,  and  no  hesitation  or 
disposition  to  error  shown.  Pink  was  five  times  named,  brown  four, 
black  and  white  each  twice,  the  other  colors  each  three  times.  She 
now  first  showed  interest  in  brown,  naming  it  as  she  played  oftener 
than  the  others,  and  once  searching  for  it,  —  "Where  brown?  — 
There!" 

The  colors  were  called,  zve  (red),  It  (yellow),  boo,  gee,  6  eb 
(orange  color),  biby  (violet),  pa  or  pil  (pink),  bow  (brown),  ba  (black) 
fa  (white). 

Though  I  consider  the  regular  tests  as  ending,  and  her  familiar- 
ity with  the  colors  complete,  at  twenty-one  months,  in  making  a 
table  of  the  total  results  of  the  tests,  I  add  in  the  fragmentary  results 
from  the  twenty-second  month,  as  the  number  of  tests  for  the  last 
color  introduced,  brown,  is  so  small.  But  for  this  one  small  addi- 
tion, the  period  covered  is  exactly  the  third  quarter  of  the  year. 


JUDGMENTS.! 

PER  CENT. 

Eight. 

Wrong. 

Right. 

Wrong. 

35 

0 

IOO 

0 

170 

I 

994 

.6 

108 

I 

99.1 

•9 

3H 

S 

97-5 

2-5 

240 

7 

97.2 

2.8 

309 

22 

93-4 

6.6 

1 1 

1 

91.7 

8-3 

117 

11 

91.4 

8.6 

63 

10 

S6.3 

13-7 

76 

52 

59-4 

40.6 

1,440 

113 

92.7 

7-3 

Pink 

Orange . 
Black.... 
Green  .. 
Yellow  . 

Blue 

Brown  . 
White  .. 
Violet  .. 
10.     Red 


Total. 


This  table  is  somewhat  misleading  as  showing  the  comparative 
standing  of  colors.  The  single  error  in  brown  counts  out  of  its  due 
proportion  in  so  small  a  number.  Orange  and  pink  were  added  to 
the  list  at  a  late  period,  when  her  power  of  remembering  names,  and 
probably  of  distinguishing  colors,  was  much  advanced.     Had  she 


'One  test  in  the  ninetieth  week,  in  which  over  forty  answers  were  given, 
without  error,  has  been  omitted,  because  my  note  fails  to  show  the  exact  num- 
ber of  answers  for  each  color.  The  most  important  difference  its  inclusion 
would  make  would  be  as  to  violet,  whose  percentage  of  right  answers  would 
become  S7.6. 


50  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

not  fallen,  when  but  eighteen  months  old,  into  the  name  confusion 
about  red  and  blue,  these  colors  would  have  been  recorded  with 
scarcely  an  error.  Apart  from  the  difficulty  between  red  and  blue, 
the  only  consistent  errors  were  mistaking-  violet  for  blue,  and  white 
for  yellow  or  pink.  Violet  and  white,  therefore,  properly  occupy 
the  lowest  place,  as  regards  distinctness  of  seeing. 

Our  impression  throughout  was  that  she  liked  yellow  best,  though 
the  only  distinct  evidence  was  that  in  her  joy  in  the  new  and  pleas- 
ing orange  tablets,  she  included  with  them  the  familiar  yellow;  and 
such  indications  as  were  given  by  her  manner  in  hailing  the  tablets 
with  joy  or  indifference  as  they  emerged  from  the  box,  were  some- 
what in  favor  of  yellow. 

I  have  tabulated  my  experiments  with  reference  to  comparison 
with  Preyer's.  The  first  impression  on  comparing  is  of  a  surprising 
superiority  in  color  perception  on  the  part  of  my  niece.  Some  cor- 
rections to  this  idea  must  be  mentioned,  (i)  Professor  Preyer 
marked  all  answers  as  wrong  that  were  first  given  wrong,  even  if  the 
child  corrected  them  himself,  while  I  mark  such  answer  right  if  it 
was  corrected  without  suggestion.  If  I  had  conformed  to  Preyer's 
method  on  this  point,  my  niece's  percentage  of  correct  answers  would 
fall  to  91.  (2)  My  withdrawal  of  red  lest  the  confusion  concerning 
it  should  become  infixed,  removed  for  some  weeks  the  chief  source 
of  error;  while  Preyer  at  one  time  withdrew  for  two  weeks  the  two 
best  known  colors.  I  have  estimated  (on  the  basis  of  the  percent- 
ages of  correct  answers  as  to  the  colors  in  question,  and  the  propor- 
tionate number  of  answers  usually  given  for  them)  that  had  there 
been  no  colors  withdrawn,  my  niece's  percentage  of  correct  answers 
would  have  fallen  to  90  per  cent,  or,  combining  with  (1),  to  88.7; 
while  that  of  Preyer's  child  would  have  risen  to  71.2.  But  the  ad- 
vantage given  to  my  niece  by  being  allowed  time  to  recover  from 
confusion  and  to  fix  the  name  of  blue  firmly  before  having  that  of 
red  again  before  her  attention,  cannnot  be  estimated  in  figures.  (3) 
After  the  eighty-fifth  week,  I  used  only  colors  of  medium  bright- 
ness, and  even  before  that  no  very  pale  tints  or  dark  shades.  The 
child's  incidental  experiences  with  tints  and  mixed  tones  show  that 
she  could  distinguish  them,  but  still  they  might  materially  have  in- 
creased the  proportion  of  error.  Again,  my  list  included  white  in- 
stead of  gray,  which  may  have  saved  some  errors.     Soon  after  the 


Shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Cliild.  51 

suspension  of  the  tests,  however,  having  heard  us  speak  of  a  paper 
sample  she  was  playing  with  as  gray,  she  took  up  the  name,  and 
thereafter  named  the  color  easily  and  without  any  error  that  we  ever 
observed.  (4)  Probably  most  important  of  all,  new  colors  were 
more  rapidly  added  in  Preyer's  tests,  thus  increasing  the  difficulty 
of  keeping  the  names  clear. 

My  niece  was  undoubtedly  much  in  advance  in  point  of  time, 
having  before  she  was  two  years  old  as  complete  a  knowledge  of 
color  as  Preyer's  child  at  three ;  and  in  the  rapidity  and  spontaneity 
with  which  she  acquired  that  knowledge.  I  credit  the  earlier  devel- 
opment simply  to  her  earlier  acquisition  of  speech  in  general ;  and 
while  evidence  is  wanting,  I  am  disposed  to  think  that  almost  any 
child  in  the  second  year  would  show  an  equal  comprehension  of 
color  if  his  language  was  sufficiently  advanced  to  test  it.  Indeed, 
where  the  power  of  speech  permits,  it  is  not  unlikely  that  tests 
would  be  better  followed  in  the  second  year  than  the  third,  because 
the  child's  independence  is  less,  —  he  is  more  amenable  to  sugges- 
tion, and  has  fewer  interests  of  his  own  to  divert  him  from  one  sup- 
plied by  the  parent.1     My  niece  may  have  received  stimulus  toward 

1  On  this  point  compare  the  notes  kindly  supplied  me  from  the  record  of  Mrs. 
Lulu  M.  Chapman  (A.  B  ,  University  of  California): — 

"  Before  the  child  was  two  years  old,  no  attempt  had  been  made  to  teach 
him  color,  and  he  showed  no  liking  for  it,  as  in  colored  pictures. 

"At  two  years  and  seven  days  he  was  shown  the  colored  tablets, —  wanted 
to  take  all,  and  showed  no  preference.  Two  other  experiments  in  the  next  ten 
days  gave  the  same  result. 

"One  hundred  and  eighth  week,  being  unable  to  interest  the  child  in  the 
color,  I  gave  up  the  tablets,  and  began  to  speak  of  the  color  of  common  objects, 
'Let's  put  on  your  blue  dress,'  etc.  He  began  to  use  the  names  of  colors  im- 
mediately, but  as  a  mere  wanton  use  of  words,  delighting  in  using  new  ones, 
without  any  reference  to  the  real  colors  of  objects:  e.g.,  he  often  remarked, 
'  See  grin  horse, '  and  the  like. 

"One  hundred  and  nineteenth  week,  I  gave  him  the  Hailmann  beads,  and 
as  he  played  with  them  tried  to  get  him  to  help  me  match  them.  He  showed 
little  interest;  confounded  red  and  orange,  purple  and  blue,  constantly. 

"One  hundred  and  twenty-second  week,  asked  him  for  a  red  bead,  —  he 
gave  me  a  yellow  one.  I  said,  '  No,  that  is  yellow.'  He  then  picked  up  all  the 
yellow  ones,  saying  with  each,  '  Yellow,'  '  Yellow; '  then  collected  the  red  ones 
easily;  then  lost  interest.  Two  days  later  he  picked  up  and  named  the  yellow 
ones,  then  grew  obtuse  and  refused  to  do  anything.     Next  day  when  I  pro- 


52  University  of  California. 


[Vol. 


interest  in  color  from-  having  always  lived  in  bright  sunshine,  amid 
profuse  color,  with  flowers  as  her  constant  playthings.  I  do  not 
think  that  our  early  efforts  to  see  if  she  could  grasp  color  names 
had  much  to  do  in  stimulating  their  use,  for  after  all  it  arrived  at 
about  the  same  time  as  the  use  of  other  adjectives.  The  usual 
superiority  of  women  over  men  in  color  discrimination  is  so  easily 
accounted  for  by  their  attention  to  dress  and  household  furnishing 
that  it  would  be  mere  speculation,  until  further  evidence,  to  conjec- 
ture that  sex  had  any  bearing  on  the  question. 

I  now  showed  her,  659th  day,  the  difference  between  light  and 
dark  green.  She  at  once  grasped  it,  and  named  them  correctly  half 
a  dozen  times.  A  week  later  I  showed  her  the  light  green,  and 
asked,  "What  kind  of  green  is  that?"  —  "Light  green."  "And  that?'- 
—  She  hesitated:  "  Black  green  —  no ! "  I  gave  her  the  word  dark, 
then  altered  positions  and  asked  for  it,  and  she  selected  it  promptly. 
I  then  put  the  dark  and  light  blue  side  by  side,  and  asked  her  which 
was  which:  she  could  distinguish  at  once,  following  the  analogy  of 
the  greens.  Two  days  later,  I  asked  her  again  and  found  that  she 
stumbled  over  the  light  blue  and  dark  blue  when  shown  them  sep- 
arately, but  shown  them  together  distinguished  at  once;  then  of 
her  own  accord  she  selected  "li  gee"  and  "da  gee"  several  times. 
Nothing  could  have  been  more  quick  and  clear  than  her  compre- 
hension of  the  nature  of  the  distinction.  From  time  to  time  since  I 
have  asked  her  the  question  as  to  red,  yellow,  gray,  etc.,  and  she 
never  failed  to  distinguish  if  the  two  were  seen  together. 

duced  the  box  of  beads  he  forestalled  all  experiments  on  my  part  by  saying, 
'  You  are  my  baby,— j»>o«  say  what  color  that. ' 

"  In  these  experiments  I  did  not  ask  the  child  directly  the  color  of  the 
beads,  but  suggested  making  a  yellow  train  of  cars,  a  red  fence,  etc.  If  he 
picked  up  the  first  bead  at  random,  as  he  usually  did,  I  rejected  it,  saying  em- 
phatically, '  No,  we  want  yellow  ones. ' 

"  At  no  time  did  he  appear  to  take  any  real  interest  in  the  color,  just  a 
forced  one  for  the  sake  of  something  else." 

Mrs.  Katherine  Slack  (Ph.  B. ,  University  of  California)  had  a  similar  expe- 
rience with  her  daughter,  about  two  years  old.  It  was  impossible  to  occupy 
her  mind  with  the  simple  color  demand;  she  wished  to  have  the  tablets,  to  play 
they  were  money,  to  do  this  and  that.  Both  these  children  were  more  ingen- 
ious and  imaginative  than  my  niece,  and  more  advanced  in  speech.  I  judge 
they  were  quite  as  able  to  distinguish  color,  but  not  interested  in  so  simple  an 
occupation.     They  are  both  city  bred. 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  53 

The  next  day  she  showed  a  curious  lapse  in  what  seemed  per- 
fectly fixed  knowledge,  by  calling  a  blue  bird  on  some  Japanese 
panels  red,  and  a  brown  bird  blue.  It  was  the  last  instance  of  such 
confusions:  the  next  day  while  in  the  midst  of  a  wail  over  a  bump, 
she  chanced  to  glance  at  the  book  shelves,  and  cried:  "Blue  book 
up  there!  Violet  book  up  there!  Blue  book  and  violet  book!" 
pointing.  The  "violet  book"  was  a  very  violet  blue,  and  standing 
next  a  pale,  unmixed  blue,  looked  still  nearer  violet.  She  was 
fascinated  with  the  shelves  for  some  time,  pointing  to  book  after 
book,  and  naming  its  color,  —  red,  green,  and  blue,  all  correctly; 
green  especially  was  recognized  in  various  indeterminate  tones.  A 
brown  that  approached  an  orange-red  shade,  she  first  called  orange, 
then,  dissatisfied,  asked,  "What  that?"  I  told  her  a  sort  of  brown. 
"No.  That  brown,"  she  said,  pointing  to  one  nearer  the  burnt  um- 
ber; "that  brown;  that  brown,"  indicating  one  after  another  discrim- 
inatingly. A  dark  greenish  blue,  exceedingly  difficult  to  determine, 
called  out  the  comment,  "That  blue."  Having  gone  about  my  af- 
fairs, I  found  her  a  little  later  trying  with  zeal  to  collect  all  the  red 
books  she  could  climb  up  to, —  "There  another  redone;  Ruth  want 
red  book."  Later  in  the  day  she  found  some  tablets  I  had  laid 
aside,  which  were  meant  as  transitions  between  the  colors,  and 
pulled  them  from  the  envelope,  saying  of  the  orange-red,  "That 
orange,"  but  of  the  red-violet,  blue-green,  orange-yellow,  "What 
that?"  Her  mother  said  that  during  the  early  part  of  the  month 
she  insisted  on  calling  a  bright  brown  dog  orange. 

Though  she  desired  the  tablets  chiefly  for  their  forms,  the  next 
day  she  occupied  herself  in  gathering  out  the  white  ones,  without 
reference  to  figure;  the  day  after,  the  white  circles  only  were  culled 
out. 

In  the  twenty-third  month,  685th  day,  I  again  noted  some  com- 
ments as  she  pulled  out  the  books.  Once,  "That  white  —  these 
white,"  glancing  from  the  book  she  held  in  her  hand  to  the  row  of 
white  ones.  Red-brown  books  she  called  red,  other  browns  brown. 
She  declined  to  recognize  a  light  blue  as  blue,  though  she  gave  it 
no  other  name.  When  I  called  a  violet-blue  "blue,"  she  corrected 
me,  "That  violet."     It  was  really  almost  as  near  to  violet  as  to  blue. 

Color  names  were  in  this  last  quarter  year  used  as  freely  as  any 
words    in  her  talk.     Thus,  666th    day,   gathering    seeds    from    an 


54  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

acacia  branch  where  both  ripe  and  unripe  ones  hung,  she  kept  up  a 
broken  comment:  —  "  Have  black  seeds.  Ruth  got  green  seeds, — 
black  seeds.  —  No;  green:  aunty  hold  down  \i.  c,  I  wish  to  have 
black  seeds.  I  have  green,  —  I  am  going  to  get  black.  — ■  No,  I  have 
green  again :  I  wish  aunty  to  hold  down  the  branch.]  —  Black  seeds 
way  up  there. — There  black  seeds.  — ■  These  green  seeds.  —  Ruth  have 
black  seeds;  aunty  have  these  [green  ones]."  So  in  the  twenty-third 
month,  e.g.,  she  begged  for  "aunty's  white  hat;"  and  as  we  drove 
away  from  a  standing  train  at  twilight,  losing  sight  of  the  lantern  at 
the  rear,  "I  don't  know  where  green  light";  as  she  failed  to  string 

fc>  o  J  t> 

a  bead,  "That  time  Ruth  didn't  get  red  ball;"  seeing  her  dress  on 
the  line,  "Aunty,  that  Ruth  dear  little  pink  dress;"  of  a  lamp  chim- 
ney, "That  get  pretty  black."  In  the  twenty-fourth  month,  e.g., 
looking  out  of  the  window  to  see  things  in  the  garden,  "Where  red 
geraniums?"  selecting  among  colored  lozenges,  "Ruth  did  take 
violet;"  seeing  a  bunch  of  balloons,  "Mamma,  see  that  balloon! 
Ruth  want  that  beautiful  violet  balloon,  and  that  beautiful  green 
balloon  too!"  (The  last  two  instances  from  her  mother's  notes, 
during  an  absence.) 

The  most  marked  form  of  color  interest  during  the  twenty-third 
and  twenty-fourth  months  was  with  regard  to  our  gowns.  She  had 
anticipated  this  once  at  the  end  of  the  twenty-first  month:  seeing 
me  about  to  change  my  gown,  she  urged,  "Aunty,  put  on  blue 
dress!"  I  brought  out  the  heliotrope  gown  I  was  about  to  put  on, 
but  she  persisted  in  urging  the  blue,  and  I  indulged  her.  She  now 
recurred  to  the  interest  as  a  habit:  the  674th  day  she  cried  out  the 
moment  I  entered  the  breakfast  room  that  I  had  another  dress  on, 
not  my  blue  dress;  and  next  day  came  to  my  room  just  as  I  was 
dressed,  and  seeing  the  brown  gown  laid  aside,  commented,  "Aunty 
need  blue  dress;  aunty  does  n't  need  that  dress  anymore."  She 
was  also  interested  to  have  me  wear  a  figured  challi  with  lemon 
ground,  which  she  called  yellow.  She  often  came  to  my  room  in 
the  morning  before  I  was  dressed,  and  at  such  times  I  usually  had 
to  reason  with  her  if  I  wished  to  put  on  a  dull -colored  gown.  The 
719th  day,  seeing  a  brown  gown  laid  out,  "Aunty  don't  need  this 
dress,"  and  running  to  the  closet  she  managed  to  jerk  from  the  hook 
a  blue  one,  and  urged  me  to  wear  it.  The  729th  day  she  had  been 
away  all  day,  and  in  the  afternoon  I  had  changed  a  brown  gown 


Shinm.]  The  Developmetit  of  a  Child.  55 

for  the  lemon  one;  on  her  return  at  twilight  I  went  out  to  lift  her 
from  the  carriage,  and  was  greeted,  "Aunty,  you  did  n't  want  your 
brown  dress  on;  you  want  your  yellow  dress."  After  she  had  had 
her  dinner,  she  returned  to  the  subject,  and  asked  why  I  did  not 
wear  my  "black  dress,"  a  gown  that  had  excited  her  curiosity  sev- 
eral times,  being  seldom  seen.  Next  morning,  after  remarking  as 
usual  on  my  gown,  she  asked  if  I  did  not  wish  to  put  on  my  "  black- 
silk'."  I  never  thought  her  preferences  among  my  gowns  altogether 
due  to  color;  she  doubtless  had  choices  as  to  cut  and  trimming,  and 
association  counted  for  something, — the  dull  gowns  were  oftcner 
worn  when  I  went  to  the  city,  the  brighter  ones  when  I  stayed  with 
her. 

Finally,  as  to  the  light  thrown  upon  color  interest  and  color 
preference,  aside  from  what  has  already  been  given  in  connection 
with  the  color  names:  — 

In  the  nineteenth  month,  the  flowers  that  interested  her  (besides 
roses,  which  were  always  favorites)  were  nasturtiums  and  fuchsias, 
—  red,  yellow,  and  purple.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  twentieth  month, 
shewas  perfectly  fascinated  with  red  gladioles  justcoming  into  bloom, 
and  hung  about  the  buds  trying  to  peep  in  where  the  color  showed. 
The  yellow  and  yellow-mottled  leaves  on  a  euonymus  attracted  her 
like  flowers,  and  she  was  very  desirous  of  picking  them.  She  was 
especially  fond  of  sweet  peas,  pink  and  purple,  but  their  long  stems 
had  much  to  do  with  it.  She  seemed  at  this  time  to  prefer  the  blue 
and  green  books  on  my  shelves,  but  size  and  convenience  of  carry- 
ing about  was  her  chief  standard  of  selection.  In  the  twenty-third 
month  she  was  often  occupied  in  stringing  Hailmann  beads;  the  first 
time  she  seemed  to  prefer  red  and  orange,  showing  no  great  liking 
for  yellow;  the  second,  she  chose  first  the  red,  then  orange,  then 
yellow,  and  though  she  gave  preference  to  balls  to  string,  would 
take  the  cubes  of  warm  colors  before  the  balls  of  cold,  — ■  when  all 
the  warm  colors  were  gone  she  took  green,  blue,  and  violet  at  ran- 
dom. This  was  in  the  evening;  the  next  time,  by  bright  morning 
light,  she  took  first  yellow,  then  red,  and  one  blue  bead  in  the  midst 
of  the  red  and  yellow.  I  thought  the  red  beads  better  liked  than 
the  red  tablets  because  they  were  of  a  yellower  tone.  A  few  days 
later,  in  still  brighter  light,  outdoors,  she  seemed  better  pleased  with 
the  violet,  green,  and  blue  beads  than  in  the  house,  but  still  pre- 


56  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

ferred  the  warm  colors.  On  another  occasion  she  gave  some 
preference  to  green;  on  another  to  orange,  then  red.  In  the  twenty- 
fourth  month,  red,  yellow,  and  orange  was  once  the  order  of  pref- 
erence, then  violet  or  blue;  another  time  yellow  was  slightly  pre- 
ferred ;  another  time,  at  the  very  end  of  the  month,  violet  was  first 
selected,  then  yellow,  green,  and  blue,  while  red  and  orange  were 
rejected  altogether.  In  all  other  cases  the  colors  were  taken  at 
random. 

It  is  scarcely  possible  to  generalize  from  these  contradictory 
choices,  but  it  is  sufficiently  evident  that  the  warm  colors  were  still, 
throughout  the  second  year,  better  liked  than  the  cold,  though  the 
difference  was  not  so  marked  as  in  the  first  year.  Yet  although  I 
watched  with  the  expectation  of  finding  evidence  that  the  cold 
colors  were  not  clearly  seen  by  her,  I  never  saw  any  reason  to  think 
that  in  the  last  half  of  the  second  year  there  was  any  material 
difference  between  her  color  seeing  and  ours.  Indeed,  her  dis- 
crimination of  mixed  colors  was  sometimes  better  than  mine,  as  I 
would  learn  by  later  comparison  with  standards.1  That  the  cold 
colors  were  duller  to  her  than  to  us  seems  likely;  but  the  difference 
from  adult  vision  certainly  could  not  have  been  great.  I  never 
heard  her  miscall  any  of  them  as  gray,  in  whatever  tint;  though 
she  did  sometimes  call  their  dark  shades  black,  especially  in  dim 
light. 


1  Bradley's  Educational  Colored  Tapers. 


shinn.  The  Development  of  a  Child.  57 


7.    Form. 

This  subject  belongs  in  the  main  under  Sight;  and  though  the 
perception  of  solid  form  brings  in  both  Feeling  and  Inference,  I  do 
not  wish  to  divide  the  topic,  and  so  place  it  here. 

Passing  over  for  the  moment1  those  primitive  observations  by 
which  a  baby  familiarizes  itself  with  form,  first  in  looking  from  all 
sides  at  objects,  then  in  handling  them,  the  first  definite  observations 
I  made  were  with  reference  to  the  confusion  of  plane  and  solid  form. 
This  was  not  frequent.  In  the  twentieth  week,  as  soon  as  seizing 
had  fairly  become  a  habit,  the  baby  would  put  out  her  hands  for 
pictures  on  the  wall,  figures  on  the  tray,  roses  on  the  quilt;  but 
seemed  easily  to  learn  what  could  be  taken  hold  of,  and  I  have  no 
farther  note  of  such  errors  (unless  her  putting  out  her  hands  to  touch 
pictures  on  the  wall,  to  which  she  was  held  up)  till  the  154th  day, 
when  she  spent  some  time  dabbing  at  the  pictures  in  a  colored  linen 
book  and  trying  to  pick  them  up,  but  never  afterward  that  I  saw; 
and  again  on  the  167th  day  she  tried  to  pick  up  figures  on  the  car- 
pet, and  the  177th  to  take  in  her  hand  a  hole  in  a  knit  shawl,  which 
looked  dark  in  the  colored  wool.  I  have  no  note  of  a  similar  error 
later  than  the  sixth  month,  except  that  at  the  end  of  the  ninth, 
271st  day,  she  tried  with  some  persistence  to  pick  up  the  moving 
shadow  of  a  rope  end  on  the  deck  of  a  yacht;  the  motion  probably 
deceiving  her  in  this  case. 

Two  odd  indications  of  entire  dependence  on  former  experience 
instead  of  direct  comprehension  of  form,  occurred  just  before  and 
just  after  she  was  six  months  old.  She  was  very  fond  of  drawing 
our  hair  through  her  fingers;  and  the  18 1st  day,  getting  a  chance 
to  try  her  uncle's,  which  was  visibly  unpullable,  scolded  with  com- 
ical disappointment  at  finding  the  close-shorn  ends  could  not  be 
seized.  This  happened  again  on  the  185th  day.  On  the  same  day 
she  was  given  a  round  cracker  for  the  first  time.  She  turned  it 
about  carefully,  as  she  was  accustomed  to  do  with  a  square  one, 
seeking  the  corner  to  bite. 

All  her  recognition  of  objects,  of  course,  rested  largely  on  dis- 

'See  Interpretation  below. 


5§  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

crimination  of  form ;  and  still  more  her  recognition  of  uncolored 
pictures,  which  began  in  the  eleventh  month.  She  never,  from  this 
time  on,  showed  any  distinct  preference  for  colored  over  uncolored 
pictures.  I  have  so  many  notes  on  recognition  of  pictures,  however, 
that  I  shall  make  a  separate  group  of  them. 

About  the  beginning  of  the  twelfth  month  her  grandmother 
taught  her  the  letter  O,  which  she  first  pointed  out  correctly  the 
343d  day  and  always  knew  thereafter;  a  little  later  the  same  day 
she  found  a  large  O,  on  the  letter  card  she  was  playing  with,  and 
held  it  out  to  her  grandmother  with  a  questioning  sound.  She  ev- 
idently recognized  the  resemblance  to  the  figure  she  knew,  and  yet 
regarded  it  with  doubt,  conscious  of  a  difference. 

By  the  middle  of  the  thirteenth  month  she  knew  O  in  all  sizes 
large  enough  to  be  clear  of  the  context,  rarely  smaller  than  bour- 
gei  lis  upper  case  (O),  but  the  383d  day  she  picked  it  out  in  the  midst 
of  bourgeois  text  (o).  In  the  next  few  days  she  was  disposed  to  hunt 
for  and  announce  lower  case  o's  in  books,  but  sometimes  mistook 
c  for  o.' 

Near  the  end  of  the  month,  393d  day,  her  grandmother  taught 
her  S,  which  she  learned  without  trouble.  Her  curiosity  and  ques- 
tions about  Q  led  her  mother  finally  to  give  her  its  name,  and  be- 
fore the  end  of  the  sixteenth  month  she  knew  it  well.  In  the 
eighteenth  month  she  pointed  out  and  named  O  and  S  frequently. 

The  543d  day  her  mother  marked  a  square  and  circle  on  paper, 
and  named  them  once  to  the  child,  who  thereafter  distinguished 
them  with  ease;  this  I  did  not  see,  but  later  in  the  day,  being  told 
of  it,  tried  it  myself]  and  found  that  she  named  the  two  figures 
readily  without  mistake.  Her  mother  had  named  the  circle  to  her 
as  "round  O,"  and  this  was  not  entirely  replaced  by  "circle"  before 
the  third  year. 

The  553d  day,  in  the  nineteenth  month,  she  called  a  roughly 
made  square  "  ka,"  without  the  least  hesitation.  She  often  asked  to 
have  "wou'  O"  made  for  her  on  paper;  and  on  the  559th  da}-  began 
without  suggestion  to  try  to  make  it  herself,  carrying  the  line 
around  to  meet  itself  in  a  long,  uneven  loop,  saying,  "Wou,'  wou' 
O ! "  as  she  did  it.  She  liked  to  see  a  square  drawn,  laughing  when  I 
began  it,  and  calling,  "Ka!"  and  "More!"  till  I  had  drawn  a  great 
many.     The  563d  day  she  started  to  draw  a  "wou'  O,"  then  began 


sinvNi  The  Development  of  a  Child.  59 

calling  it  a  "wou'  ka,"  round  square.  I  thought  from  the  motion 
of  her  hand  she  was  really  aiming  at  a  square;  however,  it  did  not 
approximate  one,  and  she  made  no  further  attempt  at  squares.  She 
continued  to  ask  to  have  the  figures  she  knew  drawn  for  her  at  in- 
tervals throughout  the  year,  and  to  try  circles  and  later  ellipses  her- 
self. 


First  attempts  at  circle,  559th  day.1 


1  In  tracing  these  attempts  for  reproduction  (with  the  exception  of  Fig.  2), 
pains  has  been  taken  to  preserve  the  character  of  the  line,  but  it  has  neces- 
sarily lost  a  trifle  in  decision.     Figs.  2  and  4  are  %  the  original  diameter. 


6o 


University  of  California. 


[VOL.  I. 


Fig.  2.     Early  attempt  at  circle,  566th  day. 


Fig.  3.     Circle  and  problematic  attempts,  662d  day. 


hlllNN.J 


The  Development  of  a  I  lulJ. 


6l 


Fig   4.     First  attempt  at  ellipses,  673d  day.    Very  typical  of  drawings  in  23d  month. 


62  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Here  was  an  interest  in  form,  and  an  ability  to  discriminate  it,  that 
should  perhaps  have  given  it  precedence  over  color  in  my  experi- 
ments; certainly  to  distinguish  and  name  a  lower  case  bourgeois  o 
in  the  middle  of  text  in  the  thirteenth  month,  was  proof  of  ability 
to  distinguish  and  name  a  circle,  and  probably  other  figures,  some 
time  before  she  could  name  colors.  My  attention,  however,  was  on 
the  color  tests,  and  I  deprecated  following  up  the  subject  of  form 
for  fear  of  making  too  much  demand  on  her  attention.  Beyond  the 
occasional  drawing  of  the  figures  for  her,  and  probably  a  few  ran- 
dom inquiries  as  to  their  names,  nothing  more  was  said  to  her 
about  it  till  late  in  the  twentieth  month,  598th  day,  when  the  new 
tablets  mentioned  under  Color,  —  squares,  circles,  and  oblongs, — 
were  given  her;  those  I  had  used  before  were  all  oblong.  After 
turning  them  over  a  little,  she  noticed  that  some  were  circles,  and 
spoke  of  it.  I  then  asked,  "What  is  this?  "  showing  a  square.  She 
did  not  at  first  understand  what  I  wished;  then  suddenly  cried  that 
it  was  a  "bi'  boo  ka!"  a  big  blue  square.  The  combination  of  the 
color  and  form  observation  spontaneously  made  on  this  occasion, 
she  found  harder  to  make  on  request  a  few  days  later,  as  related 
above  under  Color. 

For  a  few  days  after  the  gift  of  these  tablets  her  interest  in  them 
was  to  gather  out  the  yellow  and  orange  ones;  but  within  a  week 
she  began  to  select  by  forms,  and  would  pick  up  all  the  circles,  or 
the  oblongs,  never  the  squares,  and  pile  them  in  her  hand  with  the 
remark,  "Ruth  pile;"  a  method  of  occupation  with  them  that  lasted 
with  little  decline  of  interest  till  the  twenty-fourth  month.  Oblongs 
were  from  the  first  somewhat  favored  in  these  selections,  and  in  the 
latter  part  of  the  twenty -first  month,  this  preference  and  the  indiffer- 
ence to  squares  became  quite  marked.  The  628th  day  I  had  been 
giving  her  a  color  test,  in  which  she  had  answered  cheerfully  and 
correctly  till  I  came  to  the  squares,  then  had  refused  to  go  on ;  I 
then  laid  the  oblongs  away  in  the  box  and  gave  her  the  other  figures; 
but  she  began  to  call,  "Oblong!  Oblong  in  box!"  and  continued  to 
appeal  till  I  gave  them  to  her.  If  I  asked  whether  she  would  have 
squares  or  rounds,  she  would  always  say  "rounds,"  and  sort  out 
the  squares  and  hand  to  me,  but  carefully  stow  every  circle  in  her 
own  hand ;  once  in  the  last  week  of  the  month,  however,  left  with- 
out any  request  from  me,  she  took  the  oblongs  for  herself,  turning 


Shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  63 

over  the  squares  as  usual  to  me.  It  evidently  required  absorption 
of  mind  thus  to  carry  two  sets  at  once  in  her  attention,  and  she 
would  be  annoyed  when  sorting  squares  and  oblongs  to  be  asked 
about  circles  or  triangles.  On  the  633d  day  she  began  to  call  for 
the  tablets  under  the  name  of  "oblongs,"  instead  of  "colors;"  this, 
unlike  her  "red  and  green"  for  the  colors  in  general,  corresponded 
to  her  preference.  In  the  twenty-fourth  month  she  called  them 
"shapes." 

The  657th  day,  having  the  evening  before  just  renewed  her 
acquaintance  with  the  tablets  after  some  two  weeks'  absence, 
she  asked  for  the  "oblongs"  before  breakfast,  and  when  I  took 
occasion  to  give  her  a  color  test,  wished  only  the  oblong  ones  used. 
After  her  bath  and  breakfast,  she  went  immediately  to  look  for  her 
cap  to  go  outdoors,  and  asked  for  the  "oblongs"  to  take  with  her. 
Allowed  to  play  with  them  on  the  doorstone,  and  offered  a  square, 
she  protested,  "  Ruth  want  oblong."  I  gave  her  the  box  and  she 
shook  them  all  out,  then  picked  up  every  oblong.  As  she  took  the 
last  one  from  each  little  group  in  which  the  tablets  lay  scattered, 
she  would  say,  "No  more  oblong  there."  At  first  I  said,  "  Why, 
yes,  aunty  sees  more  oblongs,"  but  she  answered,  "  No  more  oblong 
there"  and  turned  to  another  group.  After  getting  all  the  oblongs, 
she  picked  up  the  squares  one  by  one,  put  them  in  the  box,  and 
suggested  that  it  be  put  away.  The  others  she  treated  with  indif- 
ference. Two  days  later,  having  asked  for  "oblongs"  and  received 
the  box,  she  at  once  chose  out  all  the  oblongs,  and  then  ignoring 
the  rest;  proposed  she  should  "go  outdoors  with  the  oblongs." 
Upon  suggestion,  however,  she  chose  out  the  circles  and  squares 
and  handed  me.  "Aunty  don't  want  oblong;  aunty  want  square," 
she  observed,  probably  interpreting  thus  my  efforts  to  prevent  the 
square's  being  entirely  forgotten.  When  I  ranged  them  in  rows  by 
shape,  she  was  interested  and  tried  to  help,  but  did  not  get  the  idea 
of  a  row;  when  I  began  to  arrange  them  by  color,  she  objected 
whenever  I  put  an  oblong  aside  and  rescued  it  each  time,  with, 
"  No,  Ruth  want  oblong." 

Two  days  later  I  asked  her,  "Which  does  Ruth  like  best?"  nam- 
ing over  the  shapes.  She  did  not  understand  the  form  of  question, 
so  I  asked  again,  "Which  does  Ruth  like?"  and  she  answered, 
"Oblong."     On   the  669th  day,  as  I  took    down  the  box    at  her 


64  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

request  for  "oblongs"  she  remarked,  "  Ruth  not  want  square,"  then, 
"Not  triangle."  And  she  did  in  fact  occupy  herself  gathering  out 
circles  and  oblongs,  —  the  white  ones  only,  though  she  had  been 
indifferent  to  color.  The  next  day,  the  last  of  the  twenty-second 
month,  she  chose  white  circles  only,  and  during  the  twenty-third 
month  collected  circles  rather  than  oblongs;  but  she  was  by  this 
time  losing  interest  in  the  tablets  altogether,  and  in  the  twenty-fourth 
month  did  not  occupy  herself  much  with  them. 

The  interest  in  the  oblong,  which  had  for  six  weeks  been  so 
persistent,  was  probably  simply  due  to  convenience  in  holding ;  the 
oblong  tablets  fitted  her  little  fist  very  nicely,  and  she  was  solicitous 
to  have  them  laid  in  an  even  pile  therein.  On  the  630th  day  she 
was  displeased  that  they  were  uneven,  and  called  on  her  mother  to 
"fix"  them.  The  634th  day  she  found  two  very  long  and  narrow 
oblongs,  which  had  been  cut  from  colored  card,  and  tried  to  fit  them 
in  with  the  others,  but  finding  she  could  not,  rejected  them  as  "too 
big."  Two  others,  which  were  smaller  than  her  set,  she  did  not 
object  to  as  too  little,  but  was  delighted  with  them,  crying  out  over 
them  as  "cunning  little  bit  oblongs!"  and  losing  them  and  finding 
again  with  joy.  The  circles  were  somewhat  less  convenient,  and 
the  squares,  with  their  corners,  least  of  all. 

Though  her  spontaneous  sortings  out  showed  that  from  the  first 
she  distinguished  the  figures  easily,  she  had  up  to  the  end  of  the 
twenty-first  month  some  difficulty  in  naming  them.  In  the  first 
week  that  she  had  them,  the  eighty-sixth,  though  she  had  seemed  to 
know  two  of  the  three  forms  well  when  drawn,  if  asked  to  give 
either  one  she  chose  it  hesitantly,  and  not  always  rightly.  The 
next  week, told  to  gather  out  the  "rounds"  she  did  it,  608th  day, 
hesitantly,  but  with  growing  confidence  as  we  encouraged  her,  till 
she  had  them  all.  By  the  eighty-ninth  week  the  oblong,  though 
the  favorite,  was  the  only  one  she  could  not  distinguish  easily  when 
called  for;  she  confused  it  a  little  with  the  square.  When  asked  to 
name  them  she  made  occasional  mistakes  in  all.  The  next  week  she 
could  find  any  one  when  called  for,  but  could  not  always  name  the 
oblong  and  triangle. 

This  fourth  figure,  with  which  she  was  much  pleased,  I  had 
introduced  to  her  in  the  eighty-ninth  week,  618th  day.     The  630th 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  65 

day,  hearing  something  said  of  triangles,  she  began  to  beg  for  "ti-a," 
and  her  mother  made  her  one  out  of  paper;  then,  thinking  it  well 
that  she  should  have  some  among  her  tablets,  a  cousin  present 
bisected  several  of  the  squares;  she  was  much  interested  in  this,  and 
understood  that  they  were  made  from  squares,  for  she  went  and 
found  another  square  and  brought  it  to  her  cousin,  asking  for  "more 
triangle."  She  also  kept  her  mother  cutting  more  out  of  paper, 
and  wished  to  preserve  these  carefully  with  her  tablets;  and  the 
634th  day  neglected  her  favorite  oblongs  for  a  time  to  gather  out 
the  new  form  instead. 

After  the  ninetieth  week  I  never  heard  her  misname  any  of  the 
plane  forms.  In  the  ninety-first,  the  last  week  of  the  month,  I  took 
occasion  three  times  after  a  color  test  to  ask  a  few  questions  as  to 
the  figures,  and  was  always  answered  correctly.  The  number  of 
answers  noted  was 

Circle  2,  Square  5,  Oblong  17,  Triangle  12. 
After  this  she  seemed  as  familiar  with  the  names  as  with  "  table  "  or 
"chair."  Upon  her  return  on  the  656th  day  from  the  two  weeks' 
absence  in  the  twenty-second  month  mentioned  above,  her  first 
indoor  interest  was  to  run  to  the  closet  and  ask  for  "oblong;"  and 
while  she  pulled  them  over  for  nearly  three-fourths  of  an  hour,  she 
commented:  "That  oblong;"  "That  triangle;"  "That  square;'' 
"Ruth  don't  want  round  O,"  handing  it  to  her  mother;  "Aunty 
have  round  O;"  etc.  The  659th  day,  as  she  collected  out  either 
set,  she  would  observe,  "  There  another  oblong.  .  .  .  No  more 
oblong?  —  There  oblong," —  or  square,  or  whatever  the  one  she  was 
collecting.  If  she  picked  up  a  wrong  one  by  accident,  she  would 
say  as  she  rejected  it,  "That  round,"  triangle,  etc.  The  forms  drawn 
were  as  invariably  called  correctly  as  in  the  tablets.  At  this  time, 
and  indeed  by  the  end  of  the  twenty-first  month,  she  could  tell  the 
letters  O,  S,  and  B  instantly  wherever  seen. 

As  the  O  seen  in  print  is  so  often  an  ellipse,  I  had  tried  to 
disconnect  its  name  from  the  circle,  compromising  with  her  name 
of  "  round  O  "  by  saying  "  round ; "  she  had  not  taken  this  up  to  any 
extent,  however.  Just  before  the  end  of  the  twenty-second  month, 
the  669th  day,  she  pointed  out  a  small  artificial  pond  in  form  an 
ellipse,  as  a  "round  O."  This  decided  me  to  give  her  the  word  circle, 
which  she  atfirst  objected  to,  saying,  "No  !"  but  soon  took  up  at  least 


66  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

in  part;  and  also  to  teach  her  the  ellipse.  She  took  this  figure 
up  with  pleasure,  and  an  hour  later  could  name  it,  "  el-li,"  or 
find  it  if  asked  for.  She  did  not  remember  the  word  next  day,  but 
knew  the  figure  very  well,  and  distinguished  it  from  circle  without 
the  least  difficulty.  She  took  much  interest  in  it,  and  kept  me 
drawing  "another  ellipse"  for  a  long  time.  The  675th  day  she 
tried  to  draw  ellipses  as  well  as  circles.  The  693d  day  I  began  to 
set  her  oblong  tablets  in  a  row,  saying  by  a  slip  of  tongue,  "Aunty 
will  put  the  ellipses  in  a  row."  She  cried  out  in  deep  concern 
"Aunty,  these  not  ellipse, — these  oblong,"  and  it  was  not  till  I  had 
made  the  amplest  retraction  and  apology  that  she  ceased  repeat- 
ing, "  These  not  ellipse,  these  oblong !  " 

The  effort  to  name  the  form  of  the  pond  was  an  instance  of  a 
habit  taken  up  something  over  a  week  before  —  the  application  of 
her  knowledge  of  figures  to  objects  about  her.  As  she  sat  on  her 
mother's  lap  to  be  wiped  after  her  bath  she  suddenly  cried,  pointing 
to  the  wooden  frame  of  the  tub,  "Cunning  little  oblong!"  There 
was  a  roughly  oblong  scar  or  spot  on  the  wood.  I  pointed  out  to 
her  another,  approximately  triangular,  and  she  called  it  a  triangle. 
Later  I  showed  her  the  points  of  a  man's  collar  turned  over,  and 
asked  their  shape;  she  said,  "Triangle."  Her  mother  said  that  the 
day  before  she  called  bobbins  "cunning  little  oblong  spools."  The 
next  day,  again  while  being  wiped  after  her  bath,  she  cried  suddenly, 
pointing  to  some  circular  figures  in  the  border  of  the  towel,  "  Round 
O  there!"  then,  looking  about  the  room,  "No  more  round  O;"  then 
as  her  eyes  rested  on  the  door  panels,  she  shouted  with  much  vi- 
vacity and  joy,  "That  oblong  over  there!"  pointing;  then  pointing 
to  the  lower  panels,  which  are  smaller,  "  That  cunning  little  oblong." 
As  we  laughed  at  this  estimate  of  size,  she  thought  it  must  be  funny, 
and  kept  repeating,  and  made  no  farther  search  for  forms.  The 
668th  day  she  drew  a  hairpin  from  her  mother's  hair  (again  while 
being  wiped  after  her  bath)  and  pulled  the  points  apart  some  forty- 
five  degrees;  then,  struck  by  its  appearance,  held  it  up  and  cried, 
"Triangle!"  A  half  hour  later  I  gave  her  a  hairpin  and  suggested 
that  she  make  a  triangle,  and  she  drew  the  points  apart  as  before, 
held  it  up,  and  announced,  "Ti-a!"  The  next  day,  667th,  she  drew 
my  attention  to  the  triangles  on  the  corner  of  a  writing  tablet,  and 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  67 

the  668th  named  those  on  the  corners  of  a  book  thus  finished  orna- 
mentally. In  the  twenty-third  month  she  recognized  the  plane  fig- 
ures with  pleasure  when  her  mother  laid  building  blocks  to  outline 
them  (673d  day)  and  later  brought  them  to  me  to  "make  oblong." 
When  I  had  made  one  she  said,  "That  big  oblong;  make  little  ob- 
long." After  it  was  made,  "Make  no  more  oblong.  Ruth  take 
back,"  and  carried  back  the  blocks.  In  the  evening  I  made  a  tri- 
angle with  my  fingers  and  held  them  up  to  her  without  naming  the 
figure.  Perhaps  because  she  was  at  dinner  this  seemed  to  annoy 
her,  and  she  said,  "  Make  no  more  triangle."  The  691st  day  she 
tried  to  put  a  circle  of  cloth  over  her  doll's  feet,  observing,  "  Ruth 
try  put  on  round  O  —  for  Tommy  feet  —  little  drawers."  The 
695th  day,  she  set  her  shoes  with  heels  together  and  toes  diverging, 
and  said,  "Ruth  shoes  make  triangle;"  then  set  them  parallel, — 
"Now  make  oblong." 

The  flexibility  and  perfection  of  her  knowledge  of  these  figures 
was  surprising  to  everyone  who  saw  her  show  it.  From  this  time 
on,  at  intervals,  she  has  always  noticed  and  named  the  plane  figures  to 
be  seen  about  her,  in  buildings,  furniture,  etc.  Yet  on  the  699th 
day  when  I  called  her  attention  to  the  fact  that  the  moon  was  round, 
she  objected.  "That  not  round  O  — that  moon."  —  'Yes,  it  is  not 
a  round  O;  it  is  the  round  moon."  She  made  no  further  remon- 
strance and  looked  at  the  moon  thoughtfully. 

Of  course  the  forms  she  observed  about  her  were  often  quite  in- 
exact; I  had  observed  from  the  middle  of  the  twenty-first  month 
that  it  seemed  even  easier  for  her  to  see  the  essential  plane  form  in 
a  rough  approximation  than  the  essential  color  in  mixed  tones. 
The  day  after  I  showed  her  the  triangle,  the  619th,  I  found  her  play- 
ing with  rough  paper  shapes,  which  her  mother  had  been  tearing 
out  of  paper;  her  mother  said  that  the  child  had  herself  torn  off  ob- 
longs from  a  strip  of  paper  and  named  them  rightly.  As  I  looked 
on,  she  picked  up  two  or  three  roughly  triangular  fragments  and 
called  them  triangles.  The  oblongs  cut  for  her  from  cardboard 
were  of  various  proportions,  but  she  called  them  all  oblongs  readily. 
That  she  could  discriminate  by  comparatively  small  differences  in 
form,  however,  was  evident  from  her  easily  distinguishing  the  circle 
and  ellipse. 


68  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i- 

Just  before  the  end  of  the  twenty-second  month,  666th  day,  I 
asked  her  as  she  played  with  some  building  blocks,  long  quadran- 
gular prisms,  "What  shape  is  that?"  The  question  was  unreason- 
able, for  she  had  no  reason  to  suppose  "shape"  referred  to  anything 
but  plane  surface;  but  she  responded  instantly,  "  Oblong."  Shorter 
ones  were  "cunning  little  oblongs;"  a  cube  she  called  a  square.1 
Showed  the  end  of  the  form  she  called  an  oblong  she  hesitated, 
visibly  puzzled  by  the  contradiction,  but  said  it  was  square.  A  cube 
cut  diagonally  she  had  no  name  for,  and  was  not  interested  in. 

Early  in  the  twenty-third  month,  673d  day,  I  gave  her  the  Hail- 
mann  beads,  —  small  spheres,  cubes,  and  cylinders  for  stringing.  I 
gave  her  the  spheres  only,  which  I  called  balls,  thinking  the  word 
simpler  and  quite  as  exact.  She  was  greatly  interested  in  them,  but 
not  at  all  disposed  to  string  them,  and  wished  merely  to  tumble 
them  about.  The  next  day  I  kept  them  in  my  own  hands,  and, 
using  both  cubes  and  spheres,  insisted  on  her  earning  them  to  play 
with  by  first  stringing  them.  With  more  trouble  than  I  had  had 
with  any  similar  exercise,  I  established  this  habit.  The  cubes  were 
easier  to  string,  but  she  liked  the  balls  best.  She  began  at  once  to 
use  "little  balls"  as  a  general  name  for  the  beads,  and  to  coax  for 
them.  E.  ^".,the  evening  after  I  had  taught  her  to  string  them, 
she  asked  for  them  again  and  was  put  off  till  after  dinner;  and 
although  the  dinner  chanced  to  involve  a  long  and  exciting  contest 
on  a  point  of  table  behavior,  and  also  a  great  deal  of  concern  over  a 
plum  that  had  been  promised  her  some  time  before,  when  it  was  all 
over  and  hands  washed,  she  ran  to  me,  laid  both  hands  on  my  arm 
and  jumped  up  and  down  by  it,  reached  up  and  kissed  me  on  both 
cheeks,  then  cried,  "Now  little  balls!"  The  next  morning  she 
asked  for  them  soon  after  breakfast.  Within  three  weeks  her  inter- 
est flagged,  but  was  renewed  by  my  bringing  out  the  cylinders  on 
the  693d  day,  and  lasted  till  the  middle  of  the  twenty-fourth  month. 

It  was  hard  to  get  her  to  accept  the  name  "cube;"  she  called  the 
cubes  "squares."  By  the  676th  day,  the  third  of  her  acquaintance 
with  them,  she  would  point  them  out  correctly  if  asked,  but  still  did 
not  have  the  word;  the  680th  day  she  still  called  them  squares; 

1 1  have  repeatedly  heard  grown  people  call  the  prism  an  oblong,  and  the 
cube  a  square. 


jmira..,  The  Development  of  a  Child.  69 

the  681st  instead  of  asking  for  "little  balls,"  she  asked  for  the 
"squares  —  no  — "  and  hesitated.  I  tried  to  get  the  word  cube 
from  her  in  vain,  yet  asked  to  point  out  the  cube,  she  could  do  it 
unerringly.  I  brought  out  the  square  tablets  and  showed  one  to 
her  beside  a  cube;  she  could  tell  which  was  which  with  ease,  and 
was  interested  in  the  points  of  difference,  and  tried  to  carry  on  my 
exposition  by  turning  over  the  cube  and  saying,  "That  got  hole  in." 
I  had  already  provided  against  her  regarding  this  an  essential  part 
of  a  cube  by  showing  her  the  cubes  among  the  building  blocks, 
and  when  I  reminded  her  that  they  had  no  holes,  she  assented. 
Four  days  later  she  was  still  troubled  by  the  word,  and  asked  for  it 
would  hesitate,  —  "Ka? — no?"  and  finally  "coo !"  with  an  effort. 
With  the  name  "cylinder"  she  experienced  little  trouble.  During 
the  twenty-fourth  month  she  asked  for  the  beads  either  as  cubes, 
or  "my  little  cubes  and  cylinders." 

The  675th  day  after  showing  her,  as  mentioned  above,  that  the 
cubes  among  the  building  blocks  had  no  holes,  I  asked  her  to  find 
me  some  cubes  in  the  box.  She  presently  cried  out  with  joy  that 
she  had  one  and  came  to  show  me  the  half-cube.  I  turned  it  all 
over  and  showed  her  its  shape,  saying,  "That  is  not  a  cube." 
"No  —  that  triangle,"  she  said, and  went  back  with  me  and  found  a 
cube.  When  I  showed  her  the  two  side  by  side,  she  said  again, 
"That  triangle."  Though  she  accepted  the  building  blocks,  which 
were  not  much  larger  than  the  beads,  as  cubes,  it  was  very  hard  for 
her  to  generalize  the  idea  of  solid  form,  and  at  the  very  time  we 
were  so  struck  with  her  ready  discernment  of  the  plane  forms  every- 
where, she  refused,  695th  day,  to  accept  as  cubes  her  larger  letter 
blocks.  I  succeeded,  however,  in  having  her  accept  an  Indian  bead, 
longer  and  slenderer  than  the  Hailmann  bead,  as  a  cylinder;  and 
the  699th  day  I  told  her  to  find  a  cube  in  her  box  of  blocks,  and 
she  seized  at  once  one  of  the  large  letter  blocks  and  said,  "There 
cube!"  then  searched  in  the  box  and  found  a  prism  that  was  of 
such  length  as  to  equal  two  cubes.  "That  not  cube,  that  oblong," 
she  said,  then  found  the  half  cubes,  saying  of  each,  "  That  not 
cube, — that  triangle;"  finally  a  cube,  "That  cube."  Finding  an 
irregular  piece  from  a  door  frame  among  the  blocks,  "  What  that?  " 
She  was  interested  on  being  shown  that  a  rolled  up  picture  was  a 
cylinder.       Once    in    the    twenty-fourth    month,    asked   the  shape 


yo  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

of  a  straw,  she  said,  "oblong,"  but  agreed  when  told  it  was  a  cylinder. 
Although  when  told  she  could  perceive  some  correspondence  be- 
tween the  forms  of  other  objects  and  those  so  well  known  in  her 
beads,  she  did  not  at  any  time  apply  the  lesson  herself,  and  though 
for  a  while  she  was  fond  of  the  beads  as  playthings,  she  took  no 
such  interest  in  solid  form  as  in  plane  form,  and  showed  no  such 
power  of  comprehending  it. 

The  678th  day  I  showed  her  that  the  cubes  stood  firm,  while 
balls  rolled  round  at  a  slight  movement  of  the  box.  Two  days 
later,  taking  a  ball  to  string,  she  laid  it  down,  saying,  "Ball  won't 
stand."  The  685th  day  again  she  commented,  "Little  balls  roll, 
little  cubes  stand  still."  This  point  she  never  forgot,  and  it  inter- 
ested her  moderately;  but  I  did  not  see  her  show  any  farther  interest 
in  the  properties  and  differences  of  the  forms.  Once,  694th  day,  I 
asked  her  what  was  the  shape  of  the  shadows  of  the  cylinders,  and 
she  answered  oblong  easily,  but  showed  no  especial  interest,  and 
never  reverted  to  the  fact  in  any  way.  Her  playing  with  the  beads 
had  no  originality,  and  unless  closely  supervised  by  me,  degenerated 
at  once  into  mere  scattering  them  about.  In  the  ninety-ninth  week 
she  tried  a  little  to  place  them,  and  also  the  tablets  in  rows,  but  she 
had  often  seen  me  do  this.  There  were  also  some  interesting  experi- 
ments in  piling  and  grouping,  but  they  have  more  bearing  on  the 
subject  of  number  than  form.  The  solid  forms,  as  forms,  were  evi- 
dently not  very  interesting  or  suggestive  to  her. 

The  last  day  of  the  year  but  one,  I  tried  an  experiment  that  I 
will  for  completeness  place  here,  instead  of  under  the  head  of 
Feeling.  I  put  several  beads  of  each  form  into  a  bag  and  let  the 
child  take  out  one  at  a  time,  telling  me  what  it  was  before  she 
brought  it  to  sight.  She  named  the  cube  three  times  right,  and  once 
said  it  was  a  ball ;  the  ball  three  times  right,  and  wrong  not  at  all ;  the 
cylinder  was  once  called  a  cube,  once  named  rightly.  The  exercise 
amused  her  much,  and  she  laughed  aloud  at  each  effort.  This  play 
was  tried  a  few  times  in  the  third  year  with  similar  results. 

I  will  add  that  now  in  the  third  year  I  still  fail  to  wake  any  de- 
cided interest,  or  any  originality  of  observation  concerning  solid 
form ;  color  and  plane  form  are  no  longer  matters  of  curiosity,  and 
her  attention  is  not  on  them,  but  they  are  matters  of  everyday  in- 
terest, and  color  especially  is  habitually  noticed  and  often  com- 
mented on. 


Shinh.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  71 


8.    Pictures  and  Other  Representations. 

Although  from  the  fourth  month  the  baby  liked  to  look  at 
pictures  on  the  wall  when  held  up  to  them,  the  first  entirely  spon- 
taneous notice  taken  of  them  was  not  till  the  tenth  month,  when 
she  suddenly  (277th  day)  noticed  with  joy  a  colored  picture  of  a 
child,  and  thereafter  noticed  it  persistently,  and  also  desired  to  be 
carried  about  to  see  the  other  pictures.1  About  the  same  time  she 
began  to  notice  and  desire  a  card  photograph  of  herself  and  grand- 
father on  the  mantel,  and  with  a  little  suggestion  kissed  them,  but 
I  do  notthink  she  recognized  them  as  representations.  Shewas  taught 
in  the  forty-first  week  to  look  at  a  picture  on  the  wall  when  asked, 
"Where  is  Mr.  Longfellow?  "  but  had  no  idea  of  its  meaning,  and 
indeed  for  some  time  confused  it  with  some  colored  figures  in  the 
frieze  above.  On  the  286th  day  she  became  confused  between  her 
grandfather  and  the  picture,  which  I  attributed  to  mere  confusion 
between  the  names,  as  she  was  but  just  beginning  to  understand 
words;  but  it  may  have  been  that  the  white  beard  and  slight  gen- 
eral likeness  had  something  to  do  with  it.  The  next  day,  asked 
for  Longfellow,  she  turned  and  pointed  to  a  companion  picture  of 
Emerson  on  another  wall,  showing  that  she  had  observed  the  re- 
semblance. After  this  she  repeatedly  showed  unmistakably  that 
she  compared  the  three  other  portraits  on  the  wall  with  Longfellow, 
but  not  two  Raphael  cherubs,  not  far  from  the  same  size.  Yet  as 
late  as  the  309th  day,  in  other  houses,  asked  for  Longfellow,  she 
would  point  to  any  picture  in  a  similar  location  on  the  wall,  but  not 
to  a  similar  picture  of  Longfellow  in  a  different  place. 

The  293d  day  I  first  thought  she  saw  the  relation  of  a  picture  to 

3  Mrs.  Beatty's  boy  was  interested  in  the  fourth  month  in  uncolored  pictures 
shown  him,  113th  day,  and  the  next  day  seemed  trying  to  get  hold  of  flowers 
on  the  lounge  cover.  At  eleven  months  he  was  much  interested  in  pictures  on 
the  wall;  the  336th  day,  taken  into  a  room  where  a  large  engraving  of  Tenny- 
son had  been  hung,  he  noticed  it  at  once,  pointed  to  it,  and  wished  to  go  near; 
looked  at  it  with  delight,  then  at  his  mother,  then  around  at  the  other  pictures. 
Every  time  he  was  taken  into  the  room,  during  this  and  the  next  day,  he  wished 
to  be  taken  to  it  and  to  look  at  it.  A  copy  of  the  Sistine  Madonna  was  also  a 
favorite. 


72  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

an  object.  She  was  shown  a  life-size  painting  of  a  cat,  and  told  it 
was  "kitty."  The  cats  were  at  that  time  objects  of  exciting  inter- 
est, and  she  now  became  excited  over  this  picture,  crying  out  as 
she  did  at  sight  of  the  cats,  and  thereafter  seemed  to  recognize  it 
without  difficulty,  judging  by  the  similarity  of  demonstration  toward 
it  and  the  real  cats.  Within  the  next  month  she  made  discovery  of 
the  purpose  of  a  smaller  picture  of  a  cat,  uncolored,  in  a  picture 
book.  The  pictures  in  this  book  had  been  named  over  to  her  as 
the  pages  were  turned,  merely  to  amuse  her,  without  effort  to  teach, 
and  I  had  no  idea  she  knew  any;  but  on  the  327th  day  I  asked  at 
random,  "Where  is  the  kitty?"  when,  to  my  surprise,  she  turned 
over  the  leaves  and  found  a  picture  of  a  cat's  head,  full  front,  and 
put  her  finger  on  it  with  a  cry.  She  could  not  do  this  again  that 
day,  but  a  day  or  two  later,  proved  to  be  perfectly  sure  of  it,  though 
other  pictures  of  cats  in  the  book,  which  were  colored,  were  not 
noticed;  she  would  turn  the  leaves  searching  for  it  unasked. 

Earlier  than  this,  however,  on  the  316th  day,  noticing  her  inter- 
est in  flowers  on  my  gown,  I  said,  "Where  are  the  flowers?"  She 
leaned  over  and  touched  them,  then  immediately  looked  out  to  the 
garden  with  a  cry  of  desire.  Next  day,  standing  near  the  wall,  far 
from  a  window,  I  asked,  "Where  are  the  flowers?"  She  leaned 
from  my  arms  and  put  her  finger  on  a  rose  on  the  wall  paper.  I 
carried  her  nearer  the  window  and  asked  again,  and  she  pointed  out 
of  the  window  with  a  cry  of  desire.  At  another  time,  asked  the 
same  question,  she  pointed  first  to  the  pictured  flowers,  then  to 
flowers  in  a  vase.  She  never  at  this  time  pointed  to  rosettes  or  con- 
ventional figures  as  flowers.  In  the  second  year,  however,  and  so 
late  as  the  eighteenth  month,  she  did  occasionally  mistake  such  a 
figure  for  a  picture  of  a  flower.  The  333d  day,  she  could  point, 
when  asked,  to  a  picture  of  a  dog  on  the  wall,  and  probably  under- 
stood what  it  represented. 

As  she  did  not  try  to  treat  the  pictured  objects  as  real  ones,  yet 
attached  the  same  name  to  them  as  to  the  real  ones,  and  experienced 
desire  for  the  object  at  sight  of  the  representation,  it  would  seem 
that  at  eleven  months  she  understood  the  purport  of  a  picture  quite 
well.  The  only  time  up  to  this  date  that  she  had  seemed  to  confuse 
one  with  the  reality,  was  on  the  329th  and  330th  days,  when  she 
offered  her  cracker  to  the  portraits  on  the  walls,  but  she  also  offered 


Shins]  Till'  Development  of  CI    CllUd.  73 

it  to  other  objects,  and  I  thought  it  a  sort  of  whim  or  play,  partly 
suggested  to  her.  In  the  second  year,  two  other  instances  occurred ; 
the  403d  day  she  bent  to  smell  a  picture  of  a  rose,  and  the  477th, 
after  asking  each  member  of  the  family  to  reproduce  a  sneeze  that 
had  interested  her,  she  appealed  to  the  portraits  to  do  it.  The 
499th  day,  also,  she  was  perplexed  by  a  realistic  picture  card,  in 
which  a  donkey  put  his  head  through  a  window  that  was  actually  cut 
out;  she  turned  this  over  and  seemed  surprised  to  find  only  blank- 
paper  and  a  hole,  into  which  she  put  her  fingers  curiously,  saying, 
"Fo!"  (hole).     She  never  tried  to  feel  or  pick  up  a  picture. 

The  twelfth  month,  birds  on  a  screen  and  a  cup,  and  cat,  dog,  and 
flowers  in  all  pictures  came  to  be  recognized;  once,  349th  day,  she 
pointed  out  a  ball  in  an  uncolored  picture.  In  the  thirteenth  month, 
378th  day,  she  recognized  a  small  picture  of  kittens,  not  more  than 
an  inch  long;  in  the  fifty-fifth  week,  without  any  teaching  beyond 
her  acquaintance  with  the  objects  represented,  she  could  point  out 
kitty's  ribbon,  kitty's  eyes,  the  man's  glasses,  etc.,  in  pictures,  and 
would  greet  a  pictured  dog  with  "bow  wow,"  as  readily  as  a  real 
one.  In  the  fourteenth  month,  399th  day,  she  compared  a  cap  on  a 
cat  in  a  picture  with  her  grandmother's  cap,  and  would  sometimes 
point  out  eyes,  hair,  and  feet  in  the  small  figures  in  her  books.  The 
405th  day  she  pointed  to  a  small,  uncolored  photo-engraving  of  a 
horse,  then  through  the  window  to  a  horse  tied  outside;  she  had 
for  nearly  a  month  been  deeply  interested  in  this  and  companion 
pictures  of  horses,  and  I  thought  from  the  first  that  she  understood 
them.  In  the  fifteenth  month,  446th  day,  seeing  a  picture  of  the 
"three  little  kittens"  seated  at  dinner,  she  began  to  smack  her  lips, 
pointing  to  the  table.  At  sixteen  months,  though  she  had  long 
known  flowers  in  general  in  any  picture,  she  recognized  daffodils 
and  violets,  and  at  about  twenty  months,  sweet  peas,  poppies,  and 
poppy  buds,  only  in  colored  pictures;  yet  by  the  end  of  the  eight- 
eenth month  she  could  point  out  flower,  leaf,  and  stem,  untaught, 
in  rough  and  uncolored  representations.  At  nineteen  months 
(578th  day),  after  being  shown  the  beak  of  a  little  dead  bird,  she 
knew  the  beaks  of  birds  in  pictures. 

From  the  thirteenth  month  she  could  understand  pictures  of 
known  animals  in  all  positions,  showing  that  she  must  already  have 


74  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

a  clear  remembrance  of  the  living  animal  in  all  these.  The  384th 
day,  given  a  new  picture  book,  she  recognized  all  the  cats  and  most 
of  the  dogs  in  it,  in  whatever  positions;  by  the  first  week  of  the 
fourteenth  month,  flowers,  trees,  cats,  dogs,  cows,  and  probably 
other  objects,  were  recognized  in  any  good  picture,  in  whatever 
color,  size,  or  position,  —  a  cow  stamped  on  a  butter  pail  once,  445th 
day.  Very  slight  resemblances  seemed  enough,  while  large  differ- 
ences were  unobserved;  indeed,  she  once  said  "moo"  at  a  picture  of 
a  camel  (445th  day).  Yet  differences  once  accepted  by  her  as  sig- 
nificant were  recognized  without  error;  thus  donkeys  and  horses 
were  easily  known  apart.  This  was  the  more  noticeable,  as  a  don- 
key, known  only  in  a  picture,  was  recognized  at  once  in  other  pic- 
tures in  quite  different  positions;  this  could  only  have  been  by  the 
analogy  of  a  horse,  yet  the  characteristic  differences  were  kept 
clear.  Her  mother  believed  on  the  405th  day  that  she  recognized  a 
barnyard  cock  from  a  picture  in  her  book;  and  certainly  a  few  names 
learned  from  pictures,  as  cow,  were  easily  and  without  surprise 
transferred  to  the  real  object. 

Apparently  human  features  in  pictures  were  not  as  easily  recog- 
nized as  flowers  and  animals.  At  a  year  old  she  possibly  knew  in 
a  general  way  photographs  of  babies,  for  she  would  kiss  a  new  one 
without  suggestion ;  yet  I  saw  her  kiss  the  back  of  the  card  once, 
365th  day.  In  the  thirteenth  month,  she  wished  to  be  lifted  to  kiss 
the  Raphael  cherubs,  took  especial  interest  in  the  portraits  on  the 
wall,  could  point*  out  their  eyes  and  hair,  and  knew  Whittier's  by 
name  and  preferred  it  to  Emerson's ;  these,  however,  were  life-size 
or  more.  In  the  fourteenth  month,  402d  day,  she  managed  with 
some  help  to  identify  the  Whittier  portrait  with  a  small  copy  of  the 
same.  Three  days  later  she  recognized  her  father's  photograph 
(the  face  scarcely  more  than  one  fourth  inch  in  diameter),  grouped 
with  eight  others;  then  after  hesitating  and  being  asked  many 
times,  her  uncle's.  In  cabinet  photographs  she  pointed  out  four 
other  members  of  the  family.  None  of  these  photographs  had  ever 
been  shown  her  before.  Early  in  the  eighteenth  month  she  recog- 
nized as  a  "  lady"  a  dim  reproduction  in  an  advertising  pamphlet  of 
a  vignette  photograph,  and  showed  it  to  us  with  interest. 

After  her  indentification  of  the  first  few  objects  in  pictures  in 
the  eleventh  and  twelfth  months,  she  began  in  the  fifty-third  week 


Shinn.i  The  Development  of  a  ChUd.  75 

to  ask  to  have  them  named,  turning  over  the  leaves  of  her  book 
and  putting  her  finger  on  one  after  another,  then  looking  up  into 
our  faces  with  an  asking  sound.  She  pointed  not  to  pictures  as  a 
whole,  but  to  individual  objects  in  them,  the  same  each  time,  those 
that  had  attracted  her  interest;  now  and  then  a  new  one  would 
catch  her  attention,  and  would  be  added  to  the  list.1  Throughout 
the  thirteenth  month  this  was  her  chief  indoor  interest,  and  afforded 
her  singular  pleasure ;  she  would  bring  thebook  to  her  mother,  grand- 
mother, or  me,  begging  for  such  an  exercise,  and  would  be  happy 
in  it  sometimes  for  twenty  minutes.  In  the  fifty-sixth  week  having 
been  told,  by  way  of  amusing  her,  the  noises  made  by  the  various 
animals  in  the  pictures,  she  would,  after  asking  and  being  told  the 
name  of  each  one,  continue  to  point  and  urge  until  its  note  was 
given.  At  the  end  of  the  sixteenth  month,  she  would  go  through 
the  book,  naming  the  pictures  herself,  usually  by  these  notes, — 
"moo"  for  the  cow,  etc. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  eighteenth  month,  she  had  a  recurrence 
of  especial  interest  in  pictures,  which  lasted  more  or  less  through  the 
twentieth  month.  The  528th  day,  turning  over  a  picture  book,  and 
coming  to  a  page  of  text,  she  put  her  finger  on  it,  and  said  decidedly, 
"  Read!  "  then  on  a  picture,  saying,  "Picture!  "  Anything  not  text 
—  any  decoration,  or  conventional  figure — wasa  "picture."  Turning 
over  the  leaves  and  naming  the  pictures  as  she  came  to  them,  she 
would  say,  "  Picture,"  if  she  came  to  an  unknown  one.  Seeing  a 
book  she  would  ask  if  it  contained  pictures,  "Picture?"  When 
pulling  down  mine  and  piling  them  on  the  bed,  she  would  stop  from 
time  to  time,  sit  down  and  open  one,  and  examine  it:  "Picture? 
.  .  .  .  Find?  ....  No?"  In  the  middle  of  the  nine- 
teenth month,  at  sight  of  a  picture  of  a  bird,  on  the  561st  day,  the 
interest  in  pictures  narrowed  to  an  almost  exclusive  desire  for 
pictures  of  birds,  which  was  for  some  days  a  passion ;  and  for  weeks 
to  "see  birdy  in  book"  was  a  frequent  appeal.  She  had  other  favor- 
ites, however,  usually  pictures  of  animals  and  children. 

Her  interest  in  pictures  during  this  second  period  of  fondness 
for  them  was  far  more  complex  and  intellectual.     The  picture  sug- 

'  In  this  way  some  names  were  first  learned  in  pictures;  but  where  possible  I 
would  always  follow  up  the  picture  acquaintance  promptly  by  showing  her  the 
real  object,  which  greatly  increased  her  interest. 


y6  University  of  California.  [Voi.. , 

gested  not  merely  the  object,  but  much  associated  with  it.  Thus 
in  the  eighteenth  month,  537th  day,  having  picked  up  a  pamphlet, 
with  the  remark,  "Book,"  she  sat  down  on  the  stairs  saying, 
"Read,"  discovered  a  small  advertising  cut  of  a  dog  on  the  back, 
and  commented :  "  Dog.  .  .  .  Bark.  .  .  .  Wow-wow-wow- 
wow-wow-wow ! "  She  stopped  to  laugh  at  her  imitation,  then: 
"Muzhik.  .  .  .  Bark.  .  .  .  Wow-wow-  wow-wow-  wow- 
wow!  .  .  .  Ruth."  (That  is  a  dog.  He  barks.  He  barks 
thus,  etc.  Muzhik  also  is  a  dog.  He  barks.  He  barks  thus,  etc. 
Ruth  too  can  do  it.)  Two  days  after,  seeing  a  picture  of  a  bell, 
she  asked,  "  Ring?"  In  the  nineteenth  month  she  showed  that  she 
understood  the  action  of  pictures.  The  564th  day  she  commented 
on  a  child  digging  with  spoon  and  pail,  "In  bucket"  (Puts  the  dirt 
in  the  bucket) ;  and  on  a  boy  with  hand  in  his  pocket, — being  asked 
where  his  hand  was,  —  "Get  purse."  A  more  complex  comment, 
but  probably  based  on  former  explanations,  was  on  the  603d  day: 
"  Ducks  swim  on  water.     Old  hen." 

By  the  time  she  was  twenty  months  old  her  greatest  enthusiasm 
for  pictures  had  passed,  though  she  has  had  a  moderate  liking  for 
them  since,  especially  in  connection  with  interesting  description  or 
story.  By  this  time,  too,  her  understanding  of  pictures  was  prac- 
tically complete. 

As  to  the  understanding  of  other  representations,  the  earliest 
ones  that  came  in  her  way  were  toy  animals  and  dolls;  and  the  first 
sign  of  any  relation  observed  between  them  and  the  objects  they 
represented,  was  that  when  her  first  toy  cat  was  given  her,  at  thirty- 
three  weeks,  her  demonstrations  toward  it  were  like  those  toward 
the  real  cats ;  I  thought,  however,  that  this  was  due  to  the  hairy 
skin  more  than  to  any  observed  likeness  in  form.  On  her  first 
birthday,  she  was  given  a  new  doll,  the  old  one  having  been  for 
some  time  broken;  the  eyes  of  this  doll  seemed  to  interest  her,  and 
she  felt  and  examined  them  with  curiosity  ;  told  to  kiss  the  doll,  she 
kissed  its  face  properly  enough,  yet  afterward  presented  the  back  of 
its  head  for  someone  else  to  kiss.  Christmas  day,  which  fell  in  the 
middle  of  her  fifteenth  month,  she  cried  "bow-wow"  at  once  on  sight 
of  a  rubber  dog,  and  imitated  a  mew  at  sight  of  a  toy  kitten.  She  had 
doubtless  understood  for  some  time  then  what  her  toy  animals  were 


Siunn.]  The  Development  of  a  Cliild.  yj 

intended  to  represent.     I  have  a  good  many  notes  upon  her  behavior 
to  her  dolls,  but  will  not  enter  them  here. 

In  the  sixty-ninth  week  occurred  a  curious  incident,  which  comes 
under  the  present  topic  as  nearly  as  under  any.  Being  in  a  dimly- 
lighted  room  with  me  in  the  evening,  she  suddenly  cried  eagerly, 
"Eye!  eye!"  pointing  out  of  the  window  to  the  sky,  where  two 
planets  (Jupiter  and  Venus  approaching  conjunction)  stood  close 
together.  It  was  a  striking  instance  of  her  quickness  to  see  resem- 
blance and  her  neglect  of  difference.  On  the  497th  day,  as  she  sat 
in  my  lap,  she  suddenly  began  pointing  and  crying,  "Baby!  baby!  " 
As  I  saw  nothing  that  could  suggest  it,  I  put  her  down,  saying, 
"Show  aunty."  She  ran  to  the  tray  that  held  the  hearth  utensils, 
and  showed  me  in  the  moulding  of  its  back  a  conventional  orna- 
ment shaped  by  chance  with  a  rough  likeness  to  a  human  figure. 
Later  in  the  day  I  told  her  to  show  her  mother  the  baby,  and  she 
came  at  once  from  the  other  side  of  the  room  and  pointed  out  the 
same  ornament.  The  559th  day  she  was  amusing  herself  by  bump- 
ing her  chin  with  the  handle  of  a  large  bronze  bell,  and  did  it  a  lit- 
tle too  hard ;  she  broke  out  into  a  wail  about  the  "mom,"  which  I 
made  out  to  be  a  complaint  that  the  "man"  had  injured  her,  the 
handle  being  a  quaint  little  figure,  not  very  obvious  as  such.  The 
563d,  she  plucked  at  tufts  of  red  zephyr  with  which  a  quilt  was 
adorned,  calling  them  roses;  pulled  out  a  scarlet  shred,  saying, 
"Leaf,"  and  struck  it  on  her  hand,  saying,  "Snap."  This  she 
repeated ;  and  several  times  afterward  recurred  to  it.  I  could  not 
make  out  whether  she  really  supposed  the  tuft  a  rose  and  the 
shred  of  zephyr  a  leaf  that  could  be  snapped,  or  was  making  believe, 
but  I  thought  the  latter.  She  accepted  at  this  time  with  pleasure 
the  shadow  "rabbits"  her  grandmother  made  her.  Her  recognition  of 
the  first  statue  she  had  seen,  the  597th  day,  as  a  "white  man"  (it 
was  in  fact  a  female  figure,  partly  nude)  has  been  mentioned. 

In  all  her  behavior  toward  pictures  and  other  representations,  I 
was  chiefly  struck  first  by  the  ease  with  which  the  general  purport 
of  such  things  was  accepted,  after  it  had  once  dawned  on  her,  —  the 
primitive  stage  of  development,  so  to  speak,  at  which  pictures  were 
comprehensible  and  interesting ;  and  next  by  the  extent  to  which 
outline  made  up  the  representation,  and  the  small  part  played  by 


78  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

size,  color,  or  even  the  shading  to  imitate  solid  form.  We  were 
never  able  to  see  that  there  was  any  distinct  preference  for  colored 
pictures  over  uncolored,  and  those  first  recognized  were  very  much  in 
outline;  before  the  eighteenth  month  she  recognized  at  once  as  a  cock 
an  impression  not  a  quarter  of  an  inch  long  on  a  white  stamp  or 
seal;  in  the  nineteenth  month  she  was  especially  interested  in  some 
old-fashioned  children's  books  with  their  small,  crude  woodcuts, 
and  little  advertising  cuts  pleased  quite  as  much  as  fine  colored 
plates.  It  surprised  me  that  she  recognized  trees  and  flowers  very 
early,  even  slightly  indicated  in  black  and  white, — the  color  plays  so 
much  part  with  us  in  the  idea  of  trees  especially.  Analogous  to 
this  was  her  calling  little  seedling  trees  "tee"  at  first  sight,  as  she 
did  in  the  eighteenth  month;  yet  the  word  had  been  learned  in 
connection  with  large  branching  trees.  In  the  same  month,  539th 
day,  she  pointed  to  a  twig  of  pink  Japanese  maple  in  a  glass,  cry- 
ing, "Tee!"  and  added,  "  Ba !  "  —  a  tree  in  a  vase.  Here  was 
neither  color,  size,  nor  surroundings  to  fix  it  as  a  tree ;  yet  in  a 
sense,  in  her  absence  of  knowledge  as  to  what  a  twig  was,  she  was 
right  enough.  The  only  instances  in  which  color  seemed  noticed 
more  than  form  were  a  disposition  about  the  eighteenth  month  to 
confuse  her  aunt's  house  and  a  neighbor's,  quite  different  in  form 
and  surroundings  but  of  somewhat  similar  color ;  the  recognition 
of  individual  varieties  of  flowers  in  colored  plates  only,  as  noted 
above;  on  the  586th  day  a  failure  to  recognize  an  actual  flower 
thoroughly  familiar  (a  sweet  pea),  in  a  novel  variety,  quite  different 
in  color  from  those  that  she  knew ;  and  the  recognition  of  an 
orange-shaped  lemon,  mentioned  above  under  Color. 

The  roughness  of  resemblance  necessary  for  recognition — the 
mere  suggestion  required — struck  me  over  and  over.  This  was 
analogous  to  what  I  observed  both  as  to  form  and  color,  and  seemed 
a  consistent  trait  of  all  her  sight  recognition  ;  it  coincides  also  with 
what  I  have  observed  of  other  young  children,  and  repeatedly  heard 
from  those  who  have  them  in  charge. 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  79 


9.    Interest  in  Seeing. 

Up  to  the  25th  day,  though  the  baby  doubtless  experi- 
enced a  certain  comfort  in  lighted  surfaces  or  bright  points,  there 
was  nothing  I  could  call  interest ;  on  that  day  her  former  staring 
at  faces  assumed  an  appearance  of  attention  and  effort,  and  a  still 
livelier  look  was  called  to  her  face  by  a  surface  of  lighted  color,  as 
noted  above.  Thereafter  till  the  fourth  month,  faces  were  the  objects 
of  her  almost  sole  attention;  in  the  fifth  week  she  began  to  smile  in 
gazing  at  them  ;  I  first  saw  this  on  the  thirty-second  day,  and  was 
told  that  she  not  only  smiled  but  chuckled  in  gazing  at  her  father's 
face  the  same  day.  From  now  on  her  gaze  was  constantly  fixed  on 
our  faces  as  we  talked  and  played  with  her,  sometimes  with  demon- 
strations of  intensest  interest,  panting  breath,  movements  of  hands  and 
feet,  and  occasional  smiles.  How  much  of  this  interest  was  excited 
by  face  and  how  much  by  voice  I  cannot  say.  On  the  forty-second 
day  her  mother  saw  similar  demonstrations  over  a  spot  of  sunlight 
on  the  white  spread,  but  I  never  saw  them  over  anything  but  faces 
till  the  third  month,  sixty-third  day,  when  she  showed  like  excite- 
ment over  strips  of  color ;  but  there  were  instances  of  an  earnest 
gaze  at  color  in  the  meantime,  as  mentioned  above. 

In  the  fourth  month,  besides  the  interest  in  faces,  and  the  few 
incidents  of  interest  in  objects  related  above,  especially  in  daffodils, 
looking  about  the  room  became  very  absorbing.  This  looking 
about,  begun  early  in  the  second  month,  had  come  toward  the  end 
of  the  third  to  be  accompanied  by  a  look  of  surprise.  This  sur- 
prise now  became  very  striking.  Held  above  one's  head,  instead  of 
showing  gayety,  the  baby  would  look  around  silently,  as  though 
absorbed  in  the  novel  appearance  of  things  (fourteenth  week  ;  but  a 
photograph  of  the  seventh  month  shows  something  of  the  same 
expression  when  thus  lifted  up).  She  would  inspect  the  familiar  room 
for  many  minutes,  looking  fixedly  at  object  after  object,  till  the 
whole  field  of  vision  was  reviewed,  then  turn  her  head  quickly,  and 
examine  another  section;  when  this  was  done,  she  would  fret  till 
carried  to  another  place,  and  there  renew  her  inspection  of  the  room 
in  its  changed  aspect,  —  all  this  with  an  expression  of  surprise  and 


So  University  of  California.  tv™.  i. 

eagerness,  eyes  wide  and  brows  raised.  The  window  and  its  out- 
look were  included  in  these  surveys.  The  habit  was  striking  from 
the  fourteenth  week  through  the  seventeenth,  most  of  all  in  the 
fifteenth  ;  it  then  declined,  but  would  recur  in  a  new  room.  Thus 
on  the  141st  day  and  about  that  time,  taken  into  the  kitchen  she 
would  look  out  of  the  window,  then  whirl  round  to  look  inside, 
and  wish  to  be  taken  to  different  quarters  of  the  room,  just  as 
earlier  in  the  more  familiar  rooms.  During  these  inspections 
occasionally  an  object,  as  sunshine  on  the  carpet,  would  attract 
special  attention,  and  even  excite  to  movements  of  arms  and  pant- 
ing. Yet  after  these  close  surveys  for  weeks,  she  would  occasion- 
ally discover  an  object,  as  1 19th  clay,  a  pink  and  white  fan  that  had 
long  hung  in  a  corner,  apparently  for  the  first  time. 

Outdoors  till  near  the  close  of  the  seventeenth  week  she  gazed 
happily  around  without  fixing  her  look  on  any  object;  on  the 
119th  day,  she  leaned  forward  to  see  me  pluck  something,  and 
thereafter  soon  learned  to  watch  objects  outdoors  as  in,  but  never 
showed  the  surprise  and  curiosity. 

After  the  fifth  month,  though  she  continued  to  look  about  with 
interest,  the  surprised  look,  eager  staring,  and  quick  turns  of 
her  head  were  rare.  The  inspection  of  her  surroundings  with 
more  or  less  look  of  surprise,  was  usually  renewed  in  a  new 
room,  or  an  old  one  from  which  she  had  been  for  some  time 
absent;  and  late  in  the  sixth  month  (173d  day)  when  I  took 
her  into  the  tankhouse  and  woodshed,  places  unlike  any  she  had 
seen  before,  the  look  of  extreme  surprise,  even- astonishment,  jaw 
dropped,  eyes  wide,  and  quick,  eager  turns  of  head,  were  as  notice- 
able as  ever.  I  stopped  by  a  glass  door  to  let  her  look  out;  she 
stared  happily  for  about  ten  minutes,  and  when  she  saw  her  mother 
pass,  going  into  the  garden,  and  when  two  of  the  family  passed 
through  the  shed  behind  her,  she  looked  at  each,  turning  her  head  and 
following  their  movements  with  deep  attention.  I  then  instead  of 
carrying  her  back  into  the  familiar  room  as  she  had  come,  through 
the  kitchen,  stepped  across  the  veranda  and  re-entered  by  another 
door;  it  appeared  to  surprise  her  very  much  to  find  herself  back 
there,  and  she  would  pay  no  attention  to  anything  else  till  she  had 
examined  the  room  and  everything  in  it  as  if  it  were  novel. 

During  the  third  and  fourth  month,  and  I   think   until  she  had 


shinn.)  The  Development  of  a  Child.  Si 

begun  to  roll  about,  when  laid  on  her  stomach,  she  always  lifted  her 
head  high,  and  gazed  around  with  a  pleased  and  interested  air,  as  if 
she  saw  things  in  a  new  aspect. 

So  incessantly  was  she  occupied  with  some  activity  in  seeing 
that  when  on  the  131st  day  I  found  her  lying  happy  and  wide 
awake,  not  looking  at  anything  especially,  I  thought  it  worth  not- 
ing; still  I  do  not  suppose  it  could  have  been  by  any  means  a  solitary 
instance. 

In  the  fifth  month,  while  grasping  to  some  extent  displaced  her 
interest  in  looking,  her  attention  to  those  things  that  did  catch  her 
eye  was  more  persistent  and  absorbing  ;  133d  day,  she  caught  sight 
of  a  brass  caster  on  a  chair,  and  remained  gazing  at  it  so  fixedly 
that  she  could  not  be  induced  to  resume  nursing ;  her  mother 
would  bring  her  face  to  the  breast  and  she  would  turn  it  back  to 
stare  at  the  caster.  Finally  her  mother  changed  position  so  that 
the  baby  lost  sight  of  the  attractive  object,  and  after  looking  about, 
at  the  fire,  the  high  light  on  the  coal-hod,  etc.,  she  consented  to  nurse 
again.  She  was  interested  in  her  toes  when  they  were  showed  her, 
and  looked  for  minutes  from  one  scarlet-tipped  sock  to  the  other. 
She  watched  people  and  things  long  and  earnestly;  134th  day  the 
whole  process  of  setting  the  table,  and  later  of  clearing  it,  without  a 
deviation  of  attention  ;  the  bobbing  of  her  little  shadow  head  on  the 
wall,  when  her  mother  began  to  put  her  to  sleep;  136th  day  was  all 
the  morning  perfectly  content  to  sit  watching  us  hurry  about,  arrang- 
ing rooms  for  guests;  during  the  whole  presence  of  one  guest,  whose 
attentions  pleased  her,  the  baby  watched  her,  hardly  having  eyes  off 
her,  perhaps  a  half  hour.  When  thus  absorbed  in  looking  at  any- 
thing it  was  almost  impossible  to  divert  her. 

The  next  week — the  twentieth  —  her  desire  to  seize  interfered 
with  her  willingness  to  gaze  long  at  anything ;  when  the  table  was 
cleared,  she  desired  the  articles,  and  wished  to  get  hold  of  her  toes 
as  soon  as  she  saw  them.  On  the  141st  day,  however,  she  sat 
about  half  an  hour  at  the  window  watching  the  Chinamen  as 
they  dug  a  trench,  and  other  sights;  and  so  at  several  other  times. 
She  was  always  interested  in  movements  to  and  fro,  especially  if  there 
was  any  bustle.  The  158th  day  she  sat  for  an  hour  without  atten- 
tion from  us,  playing  with  papers  and  watching  us  clear  out  and 
assort  a  closet  of  them.     The  sixth  month,   175th  day,  e.g.,  she 


82  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

gazed  awhile  at  her  uncle  writing,  at  her  grandmother  sorting 
eggs,  then  became  absorbed  in  the  Chinaman  washing  dishes,  and 
stared  at  the  process  in  breathless  silence  about  fifteen  minutes ; 
when  he  left  the  dishes  to  take  away  her  bath,  she  followed  him 
with  her  eyes,  so  I  carried  her  after  him,  and  she  watched  him  as 
he  went  through  two  rooms  and  carried  out  the  bath,  watched  him 
through  the  windows  while  he  emptied  it,  watched  him  back  to  the 
kitchen,  and  for  some  minutes  longer  as  he  washed  the  dishes. 

In  the  twenty-third  week,  she  would  lie  and  look  pensively  at  a 
bright  screen,  c.  g.,  and  talk  to  it. 

In  the  second  half-year,  her  pleasure  and  interest  in  seeing  was 
so  complicated  with  other  growing  bodily  and  mental  activites,  that 
I  will  mention  here  but  a  few  instances  of  the  simpler  sort. 

From  the  seventh  month  through  the  year,  the  sight  of  animals 
interested  her  exceedingly;  this  was,  however,  complicated  with  a 
desire  to  seize.  199th  and  200th  day  she  was  laid  on  a  bed  that  had 
a  high  head  with  moulded  figures;  these  held  her  gaze  a  longtime. 

In  the  eighth  month,  absorbed  and  attentive  watching  of  new 
processes  was  noticeable,  —  e.g.,  thirty-second  week,  lighting  the 
lamp  (this  interest  was,  however,  stimulated  by  her  father).  In  the 
thirty-third  week,  a  Chinese  toy,  containing  a  moving  turtle,  caused 
for  some  days  an  especial  interest,  even  to  excitement. 

In  the  ninth  month,  wheeled  in  her  carriage  into  new  places, 
she  was  serious  and  deeply  attentive  to  what  she  saw,  though  a  lit- 
tle afraid  (249th  day);  257th  day  a  spot  of  sunlight  on  the  ceiling 
excited  a  marked  demonstration  ;  265th,  she  first  looked  up  of  her 
own  accord  to  notice  branches  swaying  in  the  wind,  with  surprise 
and  interest  in  her  expression;  271st,  on  a  yacht,  once  looked  over 
the  side  to  watch  for  a  little  time  the  foam  running  by. 

Tenth  month,  281st  day,  she  discovered  with  a  cry  of  joy  and 
pointed  to  the  sunlit  tops  of  trees,  perhaps  forty  feet  tall  and  fifty 
feet  away,  and  later  (292d  day)  when  on  the  lawn,  she  pointed  from 
time  to  time  to  the  tree  tops,  especially  when  yellow  with  the  low 
sunlight,  exclaiming  with  pleasure.  On  the  286th,  she  chanced  to 
look  where  the  sunlight  brought  out  gilt  figures  on  the  ceiling;  she 
smiled,  pointed,  then  lifted  both  arms  prettily  toward  it  with  laugh- 
ter and  joyous  exclamations;  again  on  the  293d.  During  most  of 
this  month,  she  took  great  pleasure  in  standing  at  the  window  and 


SniNN.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  83 

looking  out.  She  followed  movements,  e.  g.,  sewing,  with  visible 
care  and  curiosity.  When  carried  about  the  garden, -she  was  satis- 
fied after  being  given  a  few  flowers,  and  then  was  happy  to  be  carried 
on,  looking  at  her  flowers  and  at  the  bright  beds,  crowing,  murmur- 
ing, and  laughing  quietly  with  satisfaction;  285th  day,  delight  in 
engines,  especially  in  near  approach,  was  first  noted;  this  became  a 
rapturous  joy;  and  gave  noticeable  pleasure  as  late  as  the  twentieth 
month.  At  just  ten  months  she  watched  quietly,  without  offering 
to  touch,  but  with  absorbed  attention,  sitting  on  my  lap,  while  I 
sealed  and  stamped  letters. 

In  the  eleventh  month,  interest  in  the  sights  seen  when  driving, 
which  had  for  weeks  been  growing,  became  very  marked  and  joy- 
ous; she  would  nestle  to  us  with  murmurs  of  joy,  give  small  shouts, 
lean  to  look  at  objects,  utter  syllables  in  joyous  tones,  smile  and 
look  up  into  our  faces,  clap  and  wave  her  hands;  317th  day,  from 
the  bed  in  an  upper  room,  she  looked  out  of  the  window  at  a  little 
distance,  and  overflowed  with  ejaculations  of  happiness  at  the 
spread  of  flowers  and  moving  sunlit  branches.  Again,  set  down  on 
the  floor,  she  looked  out  at  the  tops  of  walnut  trees,  now  alone  vis- 
ible, sunlit  and  moving,  and  cried  out  again  and  again  with  joy.  She 
was  much  interested  in  looking  down  on  her  uncle  from  the  window, 
and  so  thereafter.  Her  joy  in  standing  at  the  usual  window  down- 
stairs continued:  she  would  stand,  watch,  and  laugh;  the  gardener 
would  go  by,  occasionally  the  dog,  —  for  the  rest,  she  watched 
the  trees,  flowers,  and  birds.  The  325th  day  she  noticed  a  brown 
and  white  silk  sofa  pillow,  pointing  and  reaching  up  toward  it  with 
many  expressions  of  admiration. 

Part  of  the  twelfth  month  she  was  absent,  and  my  notes  are 
meager ;  in  the  fiftieth  week  she  was  deeply  interested  for  a  few  days 
in  watching  the  almond  huller,  but  soon  wished  to  get  hold  of  and 
pull  about  the  hulls. 

Of  course  there  were  all  this  time  innumerable  instances  of 
interest  in  objects;  these  quoted  come  the  nearest  to  simple  interest 
in  seeing;  and  in  many  of  these  the  intellectual  element  is  consider- 
able. In  the  looking  on  while  things  were  done  I  saw  from  the 
tenth  month  a  clear  curiosity  and  effort  to  understand  in  her  look 
and  manner;  and  from  much  farther  back  this  feeling  must  have 
been  gradually  increasing. 


84  University  of  California.  [Vol  1. 

In  the  second  year,  while  her  interest  in  sights  multiplied,  it  is 
still  more  impossible  to  separate  it  from  more  distinctively  intel- 
lectual interest.  The  pleasure  in  gazing  at  the  moon  already  related 
under  Fixation,  was  a  comparatively  simple  one.  The  principal 
interests  falling  even  in  part  under  the  head  of  Sight  during  most  of 
the  second  year,  have  been  described  under  Color,  Form,  and 
Pictures.  Most  of  her  occupations  were  active,  not  receptive.  I 
note  in  the  twenty-first  month  that  to  sit  without  an  occupation, 
merely  looking  about,  even  in  a  new  place,  seems  impossible  to  her, 
though  sometimes  —  rarely  —  she  will  remain  still  some  minutes 
absorbed  in  a  special  sight.  In  this  month,  being  once  on  the  city 
street,  she  stared  a  good  deal  in  at  windows,  and  liked  to  stop  along 
the  streets  to  see  what  was  to  be  seen.  As  an  example  of  the  rare 
instances  of  silent  attention,  in  the  twenty-third  month,  691st  day, 
she  was  silent,  watching  the  men  unload  drying  frames,  perhaps  five 
minutes,  then  went  to  see  them  dipping  prunes,  and  watched  in 
silence  perhaps  twenty  minutes,  then  began  to  examine,  comment, 
and  touch. 

Her  own  consciousness  of  the  act  of  seeing,  as  shown  by  her 
use  of  words,  may  here  be  spoken  of.  The  first  one  she  showed 
clear  evidence  of  understanding  was  look,  in  the  sixteenth  month. 
She  understood  a  number  of  verbs  then,  and  if  asked,  "How  does 
Ruth  eat?  walk  ?  cough  ?  "  would  illustrate.  The  503d  day  I  said 
to  her  idly,  "  How  does  Ruth  look  f" — when  to  my  surprise  she  ran 
into  the  middle  of  the  room,  and  thrusting  head  and  body  forward, 
chin  up,  bent  a  dramatically  exaggerated  gaze  before  her.  This 
experiment  was  repeated  several  times,  with  the  same  result. 
Within  the  month  she  began  to  use  the  word.  The  first  time  I 
heard  her  use  "see,"  was  in  the  eighteenth  month,  523d  day,  when 
she  shouted  with  joy  at  going  out  to  see  the  moon,  "Moo'!  .  . 
'Ky!  .  .  .  Baby!  .  .  .  Shee ! "  Her  mother  had  heard 
it  at  least  a  week  earlier.  She  had  certainly  understood  both 
look  and  see  long  before  she  used  them.  They  did  not  come 
very  early  among  verbs,  for  over  thirty  of  these  were  used  before 
any  verb  of  seeing.  In  the  twenty-first  month,  613th  day,  as 
she  stood  and  watched  a  butterfly,  she  looked  at  me  earnestly  and 
said,  "  Wa,"  repeatedly;  I  could  not  interpret  it  except  as  "Watch," 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of a  Child.  85 

probably  referring  to  a  story  often  read  her  in  which  a  boy  watched 
a  bird. 

Three  days  later,  asked,  "What  does  Ruth  do  with  her  eyes  ?" 
she  answered,  "  Look."  As  this  answer  had  never  been  taught,  or 
suggested  in  any  way  that  I  could  learn,  it  is  evident  that  she  had 
by  this  time  referred  the  sensation  of  sight  to  her  eyes ;  or  at  least 
had  become  conscious  of  directing  her  eyes  toward  objects  in  order 
to  see  them,  for  observe  that  she  said  "  Look,"  not  "  See." 


10.  Interpretation. 

In  adopting  this  heading  from  Professor  Preyer,  I  have  not 
used  it  just  as  he  does.  I  wish  to  include  here  only  those  simplest 
interpretations  by  which  we  translate  our  Dare  sensations  of  sight 
into  intelligent  seeing,  and  not  those  in  which  some  significance 
of  a  sight  is  perceived,  through  association  and  inference — as  when 
the  sight  of  a  cloak  and  cap,  e.  g.,  wakes  expectation  of  going  out. 

Of  such  fundamental  estimates  of  direction,  distance,  form,  re- 
lations of  bodies  in  space,  etc.,  many  instances  have  been  given 
already  in  connection  with  other  topics.  Some  comments  and 
additional  instances  follow. 

The  striking  examination  of  a  room  from  different  quarters,  as 
if  to  comprehend  the  changes  in  distance,  relative  position,  and  form 
of  objects,  was  the  earliest  noticeable  effort  at  interpretation, — very 
interesting  to  see,  and  giving  a  curious  hint  of  the  immense  amount 
of  such  cerebral  work  necessary  before  the  world  can  take  orderly 
shape  to  a  baby's  sight.  Preyer  suggests  that  the  length  of  a  child's 
arm  must  be  its  first  measure  of  distance;  I  should  not  say  so,  for 
even  before  it  can  seize  it  has  repeatedly  had  opportunity  to  meas- 
sure  the  distance  across  the  room  by  being  carried  to  or  from  ob- 
jects whose  appearance  it  is  familiar  with;  it  sees  them  in  every 
possible  position  and  at  every  distance,  and  in  the  case  of  my  niece 
these  changes  were  viewed  with  the  intensest  curiosity  and  effort  to 
comprehend  them  in  some  fashion.  Again,  this  has  a  bearing  on 
Frcebel's  suggestion  that  the  regular  training  of  the  sense  of  form 
should  be  begun  with  the  young  infant  by  the  systematic  move- 


86  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

.merits  of  forms  before  it  in  the  order  of  geometric  simplicity;  the 
fact  that  the  child  of  its  own  accord,  as  it  is  moved  about  the  room, 
takes  a  vast  number  of  observations  on  the  complex  forms  of  furni- 
ture, etc.,  and  most  of  all  on  the  living  forms  about  it,  as  they  ad- 
vance, recede,  turn,  seems  likely  to  defeat  the  purpose  of  inducting 
it  by  systematic  degrees  into  the  conceptions  of  form.  These 
spontaneous  observations  did  not  begin,  however,  in  the  case  of  my 
niece,  till  the  fourth  month,  and  might  perhaps  be  anticipated  in 
the  third  by  more  systematic  observation  of  simpler  objects  and 
movements. 

In  the  fifth  month,  132c!  day,  I  swung  the  baby  on  my  foot,  and 
as  she  had  then  begun  to  look  to  our  faces  for  sympathy  in  pleasure 
or  trouble,  she  looked  up  to  her  grandmother,  who  sat  beside  me; 
and  as  I  sat  a  little  to  the  rear,  saw  the  back  and  side  of  her  grand- 
mother's head.  Her  face  took  on  a  puzzled  look,  and  she  watched 
the  head  with  great  steadiness  till  her  grandmother  turned  and 
made  some  sound  to  amuse  her.  This  she  received  with  a  look  of 
great  surprise;  her  jaw  fell,  and  her  brows  were  raised;  and  this 
was  repeated  several  times.  I  then  began  to  swing  her  on  my  foot 
again,  and  at  each  pause  she  would  gaze  up  at  her  grandmother  till 
she  turned  and  did  something  to  amuse  her,  then  would  be  satisfied. 
I  thought  from  her  behavior  that  she  was  puzzled  to  identify  the 
part  of  the  head  she  saw  with  the  face  she  expected  to  see  when 
she' first  looked  up,  having  known  her  grandmother  was  beside  her 
when  we  sat  down.  This  also  suggests  that  when  she  gave  up  her 
eager  study  of  a  room  grown  familiar,  it  might  have  been  because 
she  had  become  able  to  identify  the  principal  objects  from  any  side. 
About  a  week  after  she  was  so  puzzled  by  the  back  of  her  grand- 
mother's head,  141st  day,  I  came  up  behind  her  grandfather's  chair 
with  her  in  my  arms,  and  looking  down  at  the  top  of  his  head,  she 
set  up  a  cry  of  desire  to  be  taken,  apparently  knowing  him  from 
that  point  of  view  without  hesitation. 

The  sense  of  touch  was  not  used  to  supplement  that  of  sight  in 
investigating  the  properties  of  bodies,  until  grasping  was  very  well 
established;  the  first  desire  to  touch  and  hold  objects  seemed  vague 
and  instinctive,  but  from  the  middle  of  the  fifth  month  to  the  end  of 
the  year,  there  was  a  growth  in  disposition  to  use  the  senses  jointly, 
with  more  and  more  definite  curiosity  and  investigating  spirit. 


sh.nn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  87 

Some  interpretations  as  to  place  and  direction  more  complex 
than  those  already  mentioned,  follow.  The  disappearance  of  peo- 
ple from  a  room,  and  reappearance  outside  the  window,  seemed  to 
cost  her  much  perplexity;  the  first  time  I  noticed  this,  was  near 
the  end  of  the  fourth  month,  when  from  a  place  that  commanded 
view  of  both  inside  of  door  and  outer  step,  through  a  window,  she 
watched  her  grandfather  disappear  and  reappear  on  the  outside, 
with  a  look  of  great  surprise  (119th  day).  This  was  two  weeks 
before  the  incident  related  above  (page  23)  of  her  correct  estimate 
of  the  place  to  which  I  might  be  expected  to  have  moved  across  the 
room,  but  that  was  a  simpler  case;  in  this,  the  intervention  of  the 
door  and  wall,  which  apparently  closed  the  view,  was  the  puzzle. 

Early  in  the  6th  month,  156th  day,  she  desired  some  flowers  in 
her  mother's  hand,  and  to  hide  them  from  her,  her  mother  tucked 
them  under  the  carriage  blanket  at  her  feet;  but  the  baby  seemed  to 
know  where  they  were,  and  leaned  forward,  plucking  at  the  blanket 
and  complaining.  Yet  it  was  the  tenth  month  before  she  became 
habitually  able  to  trace  up  objects  which  she  had  seen  put  out  of 
sight. 

Late  in  the  sixth  month,  175th  day,  in  another  room  from  the 
one  where  she  had  become  somewhat  familiar  with  the  passing  of 
people  through  the  door  and  reappearing  outside,  I  was  surprised 
to  see  her  turn  to  the  window  to  look  for  someone  who  had  passed 
out  of  the  door  as  she  watched  him ;  then,  as  he  passed  that  win- 
dow, to  the  next  one,  as  if  understanding  that  he  would  pass  by 
that.  I  concluded  that  this  was  pure  inference  from  experience,  for 
a  week  later,  the  last  day  of  the  sixth  month,  she  turned  and  looked 
expectantly  from  the  window  on  seeing  her  mother  leave  the  room 
by  a  door  on  the  opposite  side,  which  led  upstairs.  In  the  eighth 
month  she  seemed  to  understand  clearly  where  people  would  be 
seen  when  they  had  passed  out  of  the  door  first  mentioned,  and 
turning  to  look,  or  waving  adieu,  became  habitual ;  but  at  no  time 
in  the  first  year  did  her  understanding  of  the  other  doors  and  win- 
dows seem  to  be  clear. 

Her  experience  in  the  woodshed  late  in  the  sixth  month  (page 
80),  made  a  very  heavy  demand  on  her  sense  of  locality  and  direc- 
tion; and  her  deep  attention  to  familiar  persons  moving  in  part  in 
places  not  unfamiliar,  but  seen  from  a   new  quarter,  and  most  of  all 


88  University  of  California.  'Vol.  i. 

her  surprise  in  finding  that  a  new  road  from  a  strange  place  brought 
her  into  the  old  room,  shows  that  she  had  some  sort  of  precon- 
ception of  local  relations,  however  vague,  to  be  jarred.  The  expe- 
rience is  one  that,  in  a  more  definite  psychological  form,  I  can 
parallel  from  my  own  memory. 

In  selecting  the  foregoing  notes  from  my  record,  I  have  ex- 
cluded much  that  relates  more  or  less  nearly  to  Sight,  wishing  to 
keep  as  nearly  as  possible  to  the  subject  of  the  mere  sense  percep- 
tion, and  the  closely  related  eye-movements,  interpretations,  etc. 
It  is  absolutely  impossible,  however,  in  recording  incidents  as  they 
occurred,  to  preserve  a  rigid  classification  of  topics,  for  the  grow- 
ing powers  of  the  child  show  themselves  all  together  in  the  most 
complex  manner,  especially  after  the  first  year. 

Milicent  Washburn  Shinn. 


MEASUREMENTS  AND  HEALTH  IN  THE  THIRD  YEAR. 

The  child's  height  at  two  years  was  33  inches,  her  weight  28 
pounds.  At  three  years,  her  height  was  36!  inches,  her  weight 
was  set  down  at  34^2  pounds.  I  did  not  see  this  weight  taken,  and 
as  one  month  earlier  she  weighed  32  pounds,  and  one  month  later 
33 yi,  while  her  growth  to  all  appearances  was  uniform,  I  have  little 
doubt  that  33  pounds  was  nearer  the  true  weight.  Monthly  records 
were  not  strictly  kept:  so  far  as  they  go,  they  indicate  a  fairly  uni- 
form growth,  with  no  long  stationary  periods,  and  no  decrease. 

The  child's  health  was  in  general  robust,  and  her  spirits  high. 
Some  half-dozen  slight  derangements,  —  cold  or  indigestion,  —  are 
recorded.  They  usually  affected  her  spirits  very  little ;  but  a  few 
instances  of  peevishness  are  noted.  For  example,  in  the  last  week 
of  the  twenty-fifth  month,  after  having  had  a  cold  for  two  weeks, 
she  whimpered  one  day  at  trifles,  —  because  her  sleeves  did  not 
suit  her,  and  because  her  mother  brushed  her  hair  back;  and  hear- 
ing her  father  go  upstairs  from  another  room,  where  he  had  been 
occupied  without  reference  to  her,  she  whined  unreasonably,  "  I 
don't  want  papa  to  go  upstairs,"  and  put  up  her  lip  to  cry.  In  the  latter 
part  of  the  twenty-seventh  month,  two  molars  were  cut:  she  was 
noticeably  joyous  during  these  very  weeks,  running,  frisking,  shout- 
ing, and  trying  to  sing;  but  a  few  days  before  the  first  of  the  teeth 
came  through,  she  surprised  us  one  day  by  crying  clamorously  at 
trifles. 

From  the  last  week  of  the  thirtieth  month  to  the  latter  part  of 
the  thirty-fifth,  I  was  impressed  with  a  vague  diminution  of  spirits; 
the  child  showed  less  good  will,  less  spontaneity,  and  though  much 
of  the  time  she  was  running  over  with  a  desire  of  noise  and  motion, 
jumping  about,  shouting,  and  squealing,  she  seemed  to  us  rather 
noisy  than  merry,  and  we  often  noticed  a  dull  expression  of  face. 
During  much  of  the  thirty-fourth  month  she  was  quiet,  willing  to 
sit  on  our  laps,  and  said  it  made  her  tired  to  run.  She  seemed  in 
excellent  health  all  this  time,  and  her  muscular  strength  was  good, 
for  in  the  middle  of  the  thirty-fifth  month  she  for  some  minutes 
dragged  about  in  a  toy  wagon,  at  a  run,  a  large,  heavy  boy  twenty- 

(Sq) 


go  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

one  months  old,  —  on  a  smooth  tiled  surface,  to  be  sure.  In  the 
latter  part  of  the  thirty-fifth  month,  I  noticed  increasingly  happy 
spirits,  more  frequent  laughter,  and  a  charmingly  spontaneous  gay- 
ety.  The  highest  health  and  spirits  continued  to  the  end  of  the 
year  and  beyond. 

This  coincides  with  my  observations  in  the  second  year,  the 
months  of  less  brightness  being  the  same,  —  from  latter  March  or 
early  April  to  September.  Yet  there  is  no  depressing  spring  or 
summer  heat  in  this  climate,  and  April  and  May  are  not  warmer 
than  September  and  October;  while  the  summer  months  are  those 
of  her  most  constant  outdoor  life.  During  the  first  year  also  I 
noticed  a  change  of  expression,  —  a  more  bright  and  joyous  look 
and  manner,  —  as  the  fall  months  came  on;  and  photographs  of 
that  year  illustrate  the  difference. 


SIGHT  IN  THE  THIRD  YEAR, 
i.  Sensibility  to  Light. 

In  the  winter  (twenty-sixth  and  twenty-seventh  months)  the  child 
was  quick  to  notice  the  change  of  light  when  the  sun  broke  through 
the  clouds.  As  we  sat  at  lunch,  in  the  sixty-fourth  week,  e.  g.,  it 
broke  dimly  through,  and  she  shouted  jubilantly,  "Sunshine!  See 
sunshine! " 

An  instance  of  observing  and  comparing  brightnesses  occurred  in 
the  thirty-sixth  month,  when,  looking  at  the  evening  sky,  she 
observed,  "Two  stars,  —  but  one  is  darker  than  the  other."  It  was 
odd  that  deficiency  of  light,  rather  than  brightness,  should  be  the 
means  of  comparison. 

2.  Color. 

I  saw  no  decided  signs  of  preference  in  color  in  the  third  year. 
Early  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  I  thought  violet  somewhat  favored, 
and  red  disliked,  yet  in  this  same  month  the  child  would  insist  that 
I  button  my  shoes  with  a  little  red-handled  button  hook,  instead  of 
either  a  black  or  a  silver  one,  and  was  interested  in  red  leaves.  In 
stringing  the  Hailmann  beads,  the  order  in  which  she  took  them 
was  determined  by  form,  not  color.  During  the  daffodil  season, 
from  the  middle  of  the  twenty-eighth  month  on  for  some  six  weeks, 
she  hung  about  the  flowers,  saying,  "Those  your  daffodils!  "  hardly 
able  to  keep  her  hands  off  them;  and  I  was  told  that  in  making  a 
scrapbook  about  this  time  she  always  preferred  yellow  pictures: 
but  in  the  thirtieth  month  I  noticed  that  neither  form  nor  color  of 
flowers  seemed  to  affect  her  preference,  —  the  flowers  that  interested 
her  were  the  rare  or  novel  ones,  while  those  she  was  allowed  to  pick 
freely,  as  the  abundant  showy  nasturtiums,  were  treated  with  in- 
difference. One  day  late  in  this  month,  in  looking  over  many 
colored  plates  from  the  London  Garden,  she  showed  admiration 
only  when  we  came  to  bright  pink  flowers. 

Her  solicitude  as  to  the  gown  I  should  put  on  continued,  espe- 
cially if  she  was  in  the  room  while  I  was  dressing:  she  usually  ob- 

(91) 


q2  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

jected  to  brown,  and  oftenest  wished  me  to  wear  the  lemon  and 
black  challis  slip  that  had  been  her  favorite  in  the  second  year. 
Once,  early  in  the  twenty-sixth  month,  when  I  had  told  her  I  could 
not  wear  the  "yellow  dress,"  but  must  put  on  a  brown  one,  she 
asked  if  I  would  not  wear  a  yellow  ribbon,  and  turned  to  a  drawer 
whence  she  had  once  seen  me  take  one.  In  the  thirty-first  month, 
coming  to  my  room  for  the  first  meeting  after  two  weeks'  absence, 
her  only  greeting  was,  "  Will  you  put  on  your  yellow  dress,  aunty?" 
I  put  on  instead  a  dark  red  one,  which  she  had  not  seen ;  she  said 
she  liked  it,  but  when  asked  which  she  liked  best,  said  the  yellow 
one.  Except  in  the  fondness  for  this  gown,  I  saw  no  consistent 
preference  for  yellow;  and  I  think  it  probable  that  some  other 
trait  besides  color  had  taken  her  fancy  in  this  case.  Among  her 
own  dresses  she  consistently  preferred  those  with  jackets,  guimpes, 
sashes,  or  other  decoration,  whatever  the  color.  In  the  twenty - 
ninth  month,  white  seemed  to  please  her  best  among  plain  slips;  in 
the  thirty-sixth  month  I  noticed  that  among  these  pink  was  always 
chosen,  and  her  mother  told  me  that  at  the  beginning  of  the  thirty- 
first  month  the  child  said  to  her,  "  Mamma,  when  you  get  little  I 
going  to  get  you  a  pink  dress  and  a  pink  sunbonnet." 

Her  discrimination  of  color  was  so  far  complete  by  the  end  of 
the  second  year  that  I  kept  no  notes  concerning  it  in  the  third, 
except  a  few  as  to  difficult  discriminations ;  and  even  these  are  not 
a  full  record,  but  only  instances  by  way  of  example.  Thus  at  the 
end  of  the  twenty-sixth  month  she  called  a  lavender  card  "  violet," 
without  hesitation;  and  looking  at  a  very  dark  olive  book,  called  it 
(as  many  grown  people  would)  "black,"  — then,  "No,  that  ain't 
black ;  that  is  black,"  pointing  to  a  really  black  book.  A  few  days 
later,  just  at  the  beginning  of  the  twenty-seventh  month,  I  asked 
her,  as  she  looked  out  of  the  window  at  sunset,  "What  color  are 
the  hills?"  and  she  answered  rightly,  "  Violet."  Late  in  the  same 
month,  she  called  very  pale  lavender  blossoms  "white:"  but  when 
white  was  placed  beside  them  she  became  doubtful,  and  said  she 
did  not  know  what  color;  it  was  really  very  near  to  white.  A  few 
days  later,  when  I  called  a  dark  blue  book  with  a  shade  of  purple, 
"blue,"  she  criticised  me,  calling  it  "violet."  Of  parti-colored  books 
she  would  say,  "That  yellow  and  white;"  "That  red  and  white;" 


shinn  .]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  93 

once,  "That  part  brown  and  part  'nother  kind  brown,"  which  was 
right.  She  has  not  been  formally  taught  any  distinction  between 
the  many  and  various  tones  called  brown,  but  to  her  "What's 
that?"  we  have  often  had  to  answer,  "Another  kind  of  brown," 
having  just  told  her  something  quite  different  was  brown.  She 
seemed  to  comprehend  more  or  less  the  tract  of  color  covered  by 
the  word.  A  week  later,  early  in  the  twenty-eighth  month,  in  put- 
ting back  books  on  the  shelf,  she  arranged  a  very  light  brown  with 
the  white  ones;  a  light  sage  green  she  asked  about  as  if  it  were  a 
new  color,  and  when  pressed  to  say  herself  what  it  was,  answered 
"Green"  reluctantly,  as  if  she  did  not  think  it  really  was  so. 
About  a  week  later,  she  was  told  she  should  see  some  yellow  fish, 
and  was  shown  goldfish ;  she  called  them  orange,  and  presently 
asked  where  the  yellow  fish  were.  In  the  first  week  of  the  thirtieth 
month,  she  found  an  envelope  in  which  some  cards  intended  to 
connect  the  six  leading  colors  had  been  placed,  with  other  difficult 
tones;  she  wished  to  hand  these  to  me,  naming  them.  For  the 
most  part,  she  named  each  one  rightly,  according  to  the  pure  color 
it  most  resembled.  One  between  green  and  blue  she  hesitatingly 
called  blue.  "What  else  does  it  look  like?"  I  said.  —  "Green." 
The  one  between  yellow  and  orange  she  could  see  only  as  orange. 

Interest  in  color  increased  in  this  year.  The  following  incidents 
are  mere  examples  of  the  way  in  which  it  was  over  and  over  shown : — 

Late  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  seeing  a  break  in  the  clouds,  the 
child  pointed  with  a  cry,  "See!  Blue  sky!"  A  month  or  t\#o 
before  she  had  seemed  entirely  obtuse  to  the  difference  between  sky 
and  clouds  when  it  was  pointed  out.  At  just  twenty-six  months 
after  sunset,  she  pointed  to  the  west,  crying,  "  See  !  See !  Pretty 
red  sky  !  "  Late  in  the  twenty-eighth  month,  after  hesitantly  giving 
correct  answers  when  asked  about  the  yellow  and  gray  markings  of 
a  cat,  she  volunteered,  "  The  fire  is  yellow;  but  the  coals  are  black." 
Late  in  the  thirtieth  month:  "  My  trowel  has  a  yellow  spot  on  it. 
Where  did  that  yellow  spot  come  from  ?  "  Two  days  later,  she  was 
looking  silently  out  of  the  window.  "  What  are  you  thinking 
about?"  I  asked.  "I  looking  at  those  white  cherry  blossoms," 
she  answered.  In  the  middle  of  the  thirty-first  month,  she  cried  to 
me  with  surprise  that  here  was  a  piece  of  yellow  soap,  a  color  that, 


94  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

it  seems,  she  had  not  seen  in  soap.  The  same  day,  picking  up 
fallen  eucalyptus  leaves,  she  commented,  "Here  's  a  red  leaf,  —  and 
a  green  one."  In  the  middle  of  the  thirty-second  month,  picking  a 
strawberry,  "  That  is  red  on  one  side,  and  green  on  one  side." 
Early  in  the  thirty-third  month,  watching  her  kitten,  "  Miss  Gracie 
didn't  give  me  a  green  kitty.  Men  don't  make  green  kitties." 
Late  in  the  thirty-sixth  month,  "These  strawberries  are  red,  like 
I  are."  —  "  Like  you?  "  I  asked.  —  "  Red  like  my  dress  are."  In 
the  thirty-fifth  month  she  first  tried  to  compose  a  story,  which  was 
about  little  girls  going  to  school;  and  in  the  thirty-sixth  dwelt  much 
on  a  romance  about  certain  imaginary  parrots  :  the  color  of  the 
buckets  carried  by  the  schoolgirls  and  the  colors  of  the  parrots 
were  always  specified. 

On  the  other  hand,  she  seemed  all  the  year  as  much  interested 
in  small  uncolored  pictures  as  in  the  most  gayly  colored  ones. 
Once,  late  in  the  twenty-eighth  month,  she  insisted  that  some  curved 
figures  in  a  Persian  embroidery,  shaped  like  slugs  but  of  totally 
different  color, —  bright  red,  blue,  and  green,  —  were  slugs.  In 
the  thirty-sixth  month,  a  curious  incident  showed  that  she  had 
been  able  to  look  at  a  color  many  times  without  becoming  conscious 
of  it.  She  had  been  away  from  home  all  day,  and  as  we  drove  into 
the  home  grounds,  after  she  had  already  recognized  that  she  was  at 
home,  her  father  confused  her  by  asking,  "  Did  you  ever  see  this 
house  before?"  She  looked  it  over,  saying  mechanically,  "No," 
then  cried  suddenly,  "  It  is  a  red  house!  "  and  repeated  the  excla- 
mation once  or  twice;  then  after  a  pause,  "Mamma,  it  wasn't  red 
before."  Yet  she  had  all  her  life  played  about  the  house,  driven 
from  and  to  it,  recognized  it  on  return,  and  had  never  seen  it  of  any 
other  color  than  the  decided  Indian  red  she  now  first  recognized. 

Of  the  instances  of  observation  of  color  and  interest  in  it  men- 
tioned above,  one  or  two  show  something  like  a  pleasure  in  color 
that  might  be  called  aesthetic.  I  watched  for  a  disposition  to 
arrangement  of  objects  by  color,  as  having  some  rudiment  of 
aesthetics.  At  twenty-eight  months  old  the  child  began  to  take 
pains  when  putting  back  books  on  the  shelves  to  put  all  the  white 
ones  together,  imitating  for  the  first  time  the  practice  I  had  observed 
for  her  benefit  for  many  months;  those  with  white  backs  and  col- 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  95 

ored  sides  were  ranged  with  the  white  ones.  She  did  not  keep  up 
the  practice  long.  In  the  thirtieth  month,  having  her  color  tablets 
again  for  the  first  time  in  many  weeks,  she  asked,  "  What  can  I  do 
with  them?"  and  was  interested  in  putting  them  into  piles  by  color, 
which  she  had  never  cared  to  do  before ;  with  my  help  she  did  it 
pretty  well.  It  did  not  prove  a  lasting  interest,  however.  In  the 
thirty-third  month  her  chief  interest  in  the  sticks  of  the  kinder- 
garten eighth  gift  was  to  sort  them  by  colors.  Once  I  laid  them 
in  a  spectrum  of  broad  bands,  while  she  handed  me  the  colors. 
When  I  had  completed  the  spectrum,  she  surveyed  it  and  com- 
mented, "  The  green  is  between  the  blue  and  the  yellow."  Encour- 
aged, she  went  on  and  in  like  manner  named  the  position  of  each 
band  of  color. 

I  thought  for  a  while  that  some  sense  of  harmony  in  color  was 
becoming  apparent.  Her  mother  had  taught  her  that  her  pink 
sunbonnet  must  be  worn  with  pink  dresses,  etc.,  and  she  was  quite 
punctilious  about  it.  Early  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  sent  to  get  a 
sunbonnet,  she  was  a  long  time  gone  and  then  came  back  with  a 
brown  silk  cap  she  had  sought  out  instead  of  any  of  her  sunbonnets, 
saying  she  must  wear  this  because  she  had  on  a  buff  dress.  A 
month  later,  after  asking  me  to  put  on  a  yellow  ribbon  with  a  brown 
gown,  she  added,  "  No,  —  brown  ribbon."  Again,  a  month  later,  she 
pulled  oft"  my  yellow  ribbon :  "  You  must  n't  wear  that  ribbon- 
You  must  wear  brown  ribbon,  because  you  have  brown  dress."  At 
the  end  of  the  thirtieth  month  a  black  lace  hat  that  I  put  on  when 
wearing  the  black  and  lemon  slip  she  liked,  displeased  her,  and  she 
said,  "  I  don't  like  you  to  wear  that  black  hat  with  a  yellow  dress." 
This  apparent  interest  in  combination  of  colors  never  came  to  any- 
thing, and  must  have  been  mere  imitation;  and  months  later,  when 
she  had  a  fancy  for  dressing  her  dolls  in  sundry  ribbons  and  rags, 
her  disregard  of  color  discords  was  absolute,  —  no  combination  was 
sought,  and  none  whatever  proscribed. 


o,6  University  of  California.  [vol.i. 


3.   Form. 

My  notes  for  the  third  year  supply  a  few  instances  of  discrimina- 
tion of  plane  forms,  which  may  be  added  to  those  already  given  in 
the  record  of  the  second  year. 

Early  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  I  was  told,  the  child  called  a 
square  with  the  corners  cut  off  a  "round  square."  In  the  latter 
part  of  the  month,  playing  with  a  string  of  the  large  Hailmann 
beads,  she  laid  it  in  a  circle;  and  when  I  pointed  out  the  figure,  she 
took  up  the  suggestion  and  interested  herself  in  making  circles 
over  and  over.  She  then  laid  it  down  doubled  on  itself,  and  called 
it  an  oblong. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  twenty-sixth  month  she  attempted  to 
make  an  O,  and  then,  surveying  it,  observed,  "  That  ain't  quite  O." 
It  was,  as  always,  irregular;  she  now  seemed  for  the  first  time  to 
feel  this,  was  visibly  dissatisfied,  and  after  trying  once  or  twice  more 
asked  me  to  make  an  O,  and  gave  an  exclamation  of  satisfaction  at 
mine.  She  was  fond  of  scribbling  at  this  time.  In  the  middle  of 
the  twenty-seventh  month  she  struck  the  idea  of  drawing  an 
oblong  by  drawing  parallel  lines  and  then  shorter  cross  lines.  I 
saw  her  do  this  first  on  the  slate,  and  so  was  not  able  to  preserve 
the  drawing.  In  the  evening  of  the  same  day,  she  remarked  that 
she  was  going  to  draw  an  oblong,  and  repeated  the  method;  but 
drifting  away  from  her  intention  went  on  to  multiply  the  cross  lines. 
The  day  after  this  she  made  her  first  attempt  at  pictorial  drawing; 
I  shall  not  speak  here,  however,  of  drawing  further  than  it  illustrates 
her  comprehension  of  simple  geometric  form. 

She  not  infrequently  asked  us  to  draw  these  figures  for  her; 
om  e,  late  in  the  tw  enty-se\  enth  month,  she  asked  me  to  "  write  lit- 
tle oblong,"  and  kept  me  at  it  with  grave  interest  till  I  had  made 
over  fifty  oblongs,  which  she  wished  me  to  range  in  rows  on  the 
paper. 

Near  the  end  of  the  thirty -second  month,  I  gave  her  a  box  of 
the  little  colored  sticks  used  in  kindergartens,  and  showed  her  how 
to  lay  them  in  squares.  A  few  days  later  she  tried  to  make  a  square 
herself,  but  finding  it  not  easy,  wished  me  to  do  it,  while  she  handed 
me  the  sticks.     She  was  especially  interested  in  seeing  me  lay  as 


SlIINN.J 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


97 


i,  2.  56th  week.    Circle  and  ellipse. 

56th  week.    Apparently  meant  for  oblong,    yet  the  child  gave  no  sign 
hat  she  recognized  or  intended  it  as  such.     It  is  entirely  unlike  the  other  draw- 
ings or  scribblings  of  this  period. 

"-4.  56th  week.     Ellipses  and  circle,  her  usual  method  of  drawing,  —  with  a 
continuous  line,  one  figure  after  another. 


98 


( 'niversity  of  California. 


5.^5fith  week.  First  attempt  at  imitating  writing, — done  with  a  careful. 
\niggling  motion,  unlike  the  scribblings  or  figure  drawings,  while  she  said 
slowly,  as  she  had  heard  us  say  in  writing  her  name  to  amuse  her,  "  R-u  t-h." 
MO1^  6.-5Sth  week.  This  figure  began  with  the  nimless  scribbling  (see  Fig.  7) 
seen  in  the  lower  part;  but  having  accidentally  discovered  a  triangle  amid  her 
lines,  the  child  began  to  alter  their  character,  in  order  to  produce  more  tri- 
angles; and  later  scribblings  took  more  the  character  of  the  upper  part  of  this 
figure,  and  triangles  were  from  time  to  time  sought  among  them. 


11° 

7-  "jSth  week.     Typical  aimless  scribbling  of  that  date. 
I  i  J  8.  Sist  week.     Having  made  the  four-cornered  figure,  the  child  cried  out 
that  it  was  a  square.      It  may  have   been   accidentally  made.      In   the   two 
attempts  at  circles  intersecting  it,  note  how   nearly  the   line   comes   back  to 
meet  itself. 

The  child  was  entirely  indifferent  to  making  her  figures  clear  and  sepa- 
rate, and  constantly  drew  them  over  each  other,  as  in  this  instance.  As  origi- 
nally drawn,  several  other  figures  crossed  these  three. 

Uij  9.  HSsd  week.  The  child,  scribbling,  cried  to  me  that  she  had  made  an 
oblong,  and  showed  me  the  upper  figure  (A)  in  this  group.  The  other  figures 
in  it  were  all  made  in  attempts  to  repeat  the  oblong.  After  making  B,  she  ex- 
claimed that  it  was  a  triangle. 

10.  t$4  week,  oblong. 
(too) 

llbtfc 


-MIINN.] 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


101 


/S"2«. 

ir.  Square,  oblong,  ellipse  and  circle,  §oth  week 
The  inferiority  of  forms  drawn  in  the  vhirty -fifth  month  to  those  drawn 
earlier  seems  partly  due  to  the  fact  that  she  had  not  been  handling  the  tablets 
for  some  time;  partly  to  more  consciousness  and  effort,  —  she  drew  with  less 
free  hand. 

That  the  difference  between  square  and  oblong,  circle  and  ellipse,  was  dis 
tinct  in  her  mind  before  she  began  to  draw  was  evident  by  the  way  she  went  to 
work. 


I02  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

large  squares  as  possible,  or  else  one  within  another  with  sides 
parallel.  She  was  occupied  at  least  an  hour  thus,  and  for  some 
weeks  took  more  or  less  interest  in  seeing  forms  made  with  sticks. 
About  the  middle  of  the  thirty-third  month,  making  a  square  for 
her,  I  asked  her  as  usual  to  hand  me  four  sticks.  She  selected  four 
of  the  longest  ones,  then  handed  me  another,  saying,  "  Take  five 
sticks,  aunty;  make  me  a  big,  big  square."  When  I  told  her  I 
could  not  make  her  a  square  with  five,  she  seemed  utterly  uncom- 
prehending. I  then  showed  her  the  figure  they  made,  and  had  her 
count  the  sides;  she  admitted  that  it  was  not  a  square,  but  was 
silent  and  seemed  puzzled. 

She  did  not  show  the  marked  interest  of  the  second  year  in 
recognizing  plane  forms  about  her,  but  used  their  names  easily  on 
occasion  ;  e.  g.,  she  spoke  of  the  stamp  in  the  corner  of  a  sheet  of 
commercial  note  paper  as  "that  ellipse." 

A  few  notes  on  solid  form  also  were  kept  during  the  year.  Early 
in  the  twenty- fifth  month  the  child  called  a  rectangular  prism  an 
"oblong  cylinder."  Late  in  the  twenty-sixth  she  accepted  readily 
a  description  of  a  stovepipe  as  a  cylinder;  and  by  the  twenty-ninth 
month  I  thought  she  had  the  idea  of  the  cylinder,  and  still  more  of 
the  ball,  fairly  well  generalized,  —  though  nothing  like  as  well  as 
that  of  the  plane  figures.  The  cube  she  could  not  recognize  except 
in  the  blocks  and  Hailmann  beads  with  which  she  had  been  taught; 
a  cubical  box,  for  instance,  never  suggested  the  cube  to  her.  Indeed, 
I  have  neither  note  nor  memory  of  ever  hearing  her  spontaneously 
recognize  any  solid  form  in  the  objects  about  her.  In  the  middle  of 
the  thirtieth  month  I  once  asked  her  what  shape  her  balloon  was. 
"I  don't  know."  —  "Cube,  or  cylinder,  or  ball?"  I  asked.  —  "Not 
cube  and  cylinder,  —  ball,  aunty."  The  same  day  I  gave  her  the 
kindergarten  "second  gift,"  and  in  this  she  recognized  the  three 
forms  at  once.  About  the  middle  of  the  thirty-fifth  month,  I  asked 
her  what  shape  her  arm  was;  she  considered  carefully,  and  said  it 
was  a  cylinder.  Next  day  I  asked  her  the  shape  of  the  branches  of 
a  tree  in  which  the  typical  shape  is  unusually  apparent,  and  again  after 
consideration  she  answered,  "Cylinder."  In  the  thirty-sixth  month 
I  cautioned  her,  "If  you  should  drop  your  ball  out  of  the  buggy, 
the  wheel  might  run  over  it."  —  "And  make  it  a  cy'der,"  she  said, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  103 

—  a  somewhat  remarkable  suggestion,  as  she  had  not  been  told 
anything  like  this.  (Even  in  the  fourth  year,  her  idea  of  the  cylin- 
der is  not  altogether  general;  for  when  she  was  about  thirty-nine 
months  old  someone  asked  her  the  shape  of  some  cylindrical  blocks 
shorter  than  their  diameter,  a  novel  form  to  her,  and  she  at  once 
resorted  to  the  test  of  rolling  them,  and  finding  they  would  roll, 
answered  that  they  were  cylinders.) 

Two  or  three  times  in  the  twenty-fifth  and  twenty-sixth  months 
she  selected  the  Hailmann  beads  according  to  form  by  their  feeling, 
as  in  the  second  year,  without  difficulty. 

At  the  beginning  of  the  year,  in  stringing  and  playing  with  the 
beads,  she  preferred  the  cylinders  and  cared  least  for  the  cubes; 
though  she  asked  for  the  beads  under  the  name  of  "cubes."  In  a 
few  days,  however,  she  began  to  ask  for  them  as  "my  little  cubes 
and  cylinders."  During  the  twenty-sixth  month  she  would  almost 
always  select  and  lay  aside,  or  hand  to  me,  the  balls,  then  amuse 
herself  with  the  rest;  this  probably  because  the  balls  were  trouble- 
some in  rolling  away,  and  could  not  be  piled.  Up  to  the  middle 
of  this  month  she  took  much  interest  in  the  beads,  sometimes  string- 
ing them,  sometimes  piling,  sometimes  assorting  a  little;  after  this 
she  ceased  to  care  much  for  them;  in  the  latter  part  of  the  twenty- 
sixth  month,  however,  she  played  with  them  again  for  several  days, 
and  in  stringing  made  unmistakable  choice  of  balls  first,  then  cylin- 
ders, then  cubes,  — ■  once  saying  as  she  began,  "First  the  balls,  then 
the  cylinders,  then  the  cubes." 


4.  Pictures. 

I  have  frequent  notes  of  interest  in  pictures  during  the  early 
months  of  the  year,  but  none  from  the  thirtieth  to  the  thirty-fifth 
(April  —  July).  In  the  fall  months,  the  thirty-fifth  and  thirty-sixth, 
the  interest  revived.  The  freedom  of  outdoor  occupation  during  the 
summer  months  may  have  had  to  do  with  the  loss  of  interest,  but  I 
cannot  but  note  its  coincidence  with  the  period  of  lowered  mental, 
or  temperamental,  brightness  noted  above. 

Pictures  of  animals  were  oftenest  interesting;  in  the  twenty- 
sixth  month  a  book  with  fine  plates  of  birds  and  flowers  was  for 


104  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

some  days  a  favorite,  attention  being  given  entirely  to  the  birds; 
later  in  the  same  month,  a  jeweler's  catalogue  was  most  interesting, 
and  the  uncolored  cuts  of  watches,  rings,  pins,  etc.,  were  pored 
over  day  after  day  with  many  questions;  in  the  twenty-eighth  month 
a  German  picture  book  with  a  variety  of  pictures,  all  figures,  and  all 
telling  some  story,  was  favored;  in  the  thirty -fifth  and  thirty-sixth 
•^  ^-£x/>-v^  months,  Ploss's  "Kindcrpflege"  with  its  small  uncolored  cuts  of 
~~U^sL^cx,  babies'  cribs,  chairs,  the  methods  of  carrying  them,  etc.,  in  various 
countries,  occupied  and  interested  the  child  deeply.  I  have  men- 
tioned above  my  observation  that  small,  uncolored  pictures  pleased 
her  as  well  as  the  finest  ones :  the  story  told  seemed  the  thing  she 
cared  for.  That  is  to  say,  pictures  were  still  merely  a  language  to 
her;  there  was  no  evidence  of  an  aesthetic  side  to  her  interest.  She 
liked  to  be  told  a  little  about  each  picture,  but  not  too  much,  —  the 
story  must  be  mainly  told  by  the  picture. 

I  cannot  doubt  that  in  this  third  year  she  understood  well  enough 
the  relation  of  pictures  to  real  objects;  yet  once  in  the  latter  part  of 
the  twenty-fifth  month,  in  a  movement  of  sympathy  for  a  lamb 
caught  in  briars,  she  tried  to  lift  a  branch  that  lay  across  him  in  the 
picture,  —  whether  forgetting  for  the  moment  that  she  could  not, 
or  merely  as  a  demonstration  of  feeling.  Again,  in  the  last  week'  of 
the  thirty -fifth  month,  looking  at  a  picture  of  a  chamois  defending 
her  little  one  from  an  eagle,  she  asked  anxiously  if  the  mamma 
would  drive  the  eagle  away,  and  presently,  quite  simply  and  uncon- 
sciously, placed  her  little  hand  edgewise  on  the  picture,  so  as  to 
make  a  fence  between  the  eagle  and  the  chamois. 

Early  in  the  twenty-sixth  month,  looking  at  the  watches  in  the 
jeweler's  catalogue  she  said,  "Aunty,  that  some  kind  of  a  watch  — 
to  turn  round  and  round"  (as  she  had  seen  stem-winders  wound). 
When  a  picture  of  a  breaking  lamp  was  explained  to  her,  she  looked 
at  it  seriously,  then  said,  "That  lamp  chimney  did  broke."  With 
these  simple  interpretations  compare  one  of  about  the  middle  of  the 
twenty-seventh  month :  "When  Prudy  has  given  that  little  leaf  to 
that  bird,"  - —  pointing,  —  "then  she  will  get  down  and  play  with 
that  kitty-cat." 

At  twenty-six  months  she  recognized  from  pictures,  after  a  little 
consideration,  a  small  carved  elephant,  about  two  feet  high.  In  the 
last  week  of  the  year,  she  recognized  the  first  living  elephant  and 


shinn]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  105 

monkeys  she  had  seen,  and  I  think  recognized  camels  also,  all  from 
pictures.     Evidently  size  counted  for  little  in  her  recognition.1 

5.   Interest. 

Interest  in  seeing  for  its  own  sake,  apart  from  some  pretty  dis- 
tinct mental  occupation,  was  very  rare  in  the  third  year.  To  sit  and 
gaze  quietly  at  anything  was  almost  impossible  to  the  child.  Late 
in  the  twenty-eighth  month  she  once  sat  quietly  in  my  lap  gazing 
out  of  the  window,  which  my  note  comments  on  as  "most  unusual"; 
and  again  near  the  end  of  the  thirtieth  month.  This  second  time  I 
asked  her,  "What  are  you  thinking  about?"  —  "I  looking  at  those 
white  cherry  blossoms,"  —  a  tree  about  150  feet  away.  After  a 
pause:  "And  I  looking  at  those  little  baby  roses,"  —  a  Banksia  in 
heavy  bloom.  Once,  early  in  the  thirty-fifth  month,  I  note  her  lik- 
ing for  sitting  with  me  at  the  window  and  looking  out  at  the  stars 
and  dark  trees.  She  was  also  always  fond  of  being  carried  into  the 
garden  to  see  the  moon,  and  had  at  such  times  a  pleased  but  some- 
what hushed  and  awed  manner. 

Besides  what  may  be  found  in  these  incidents  and  those  under 
Color,  above,  I  saw  little  trace  of  aesthetic  sensibility.  The  crude 
liking  for  ornamented  dress  seemed  its  most  clear  and  consistent 
exhibition.  Once  in  the  thirty-fifth  month  I  was  told  that  the  child 
displayed  very  unusual  temper  in  the  intensity  of  her  desire  to  wear 
one  of  the  favored  dresses,  and  resisted  having  a  plain  one  put  on, 
with  tears  and  outcry:  "I  will  unbutton  dis  and  take  it  off! "  "I 
don't  sink  it  is  pitty  !  " 

Some  instances  of  expressions  of  admiration  (not  a  frequent 
thing  with  her)  would  be  more  significant  if  it  were  possible  to  know 
how  soon  such  words  as  "pretty,"  "beautiful,"  came  to  mean  any- 
thing more  to  her  than  merely  interesting  or  novel ;  or  how  far  they 
were  used  in  sheer  imitation.  Thus  near  the  end  of  the  twenty-fifth 
month,  she  said,  "How  pretty  that  is!"  of  a  brooch;  in  the  twenty- 
seventh  month,  taken  out  on  Christmas  eve  for  a  drive  to  an  aunt's, 
she  cried  joyously,  "O  beautiful  moon  !  and  star! "  [O  boo'fu'  moo' ! 

'That  she  could  estimate  size  somewhat  by  the  eye  was  evident  on  this  occa- 
sion: for  the  day  after  she  saw  these  animals,  I  asked  her  how  big  the  elephant 
was,  —  as  big  as  grandpa's  house? — "Oh,  no!"  she  cried,  as  if  at  an  absurdity 
—  "Bigger  than  grandpa's  horses?  "  —  "Oh,  yes  !  " 


106  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

a'  'tar!]  as  soon  as  she  saw  them.  Late  in  the  thirtieth  month, 
"How  pretty  that  tree  is!"  late  in  the  thirty-first,  "I  like  to  see  so 
many  roses!"  at  just  thirty-one  months  old,  driving  over  a  bridge, 
"See  the  beautifully  creek !  "  [de  booful-ly  keek] ;  in  the  thirty-fourth 
month,  "I*  ve  got  the  prettiest  stone !  "  In  the  thirty-fifth  month  she 
said  of  the  doll  she  preferred,  "I  think  this  one  is  the  prettiest;" 
again,  "I  like  her  best;"  again,  "I  like  her  very  much,  because  I 
love  her  so."  The  doll  was  not  the  prettiest,  nor  even  the  freshest, 
but  an  old  cast-off  one  that  had  come  into  the  child's  hands;  and 
was  probably  preferred  simply  because  the  most  lately  acquired. 
In  the  thirty-sixth  month  she  was  pleased  with  a  book  with  conspic- 
uous cover  decoration,  and  cried,  "Here's  a  book  wiv  some  pitty 
f 'oways  [flowers]  on  it !  " 


HEARING, 
i.   Sensibility  to  Sound. 

The  first  distinct  evidence  of  hearing  that  I  myself  observed 
was  on  the  6th  day:  the  baby  cried  out  suddenly  in  her  sleep  when 
her  father  tore  a  paper  sharply  some  four  or  five  feet  from  her.  I 
was  told  however  that  earlier,  on  the  3d  or  4th  day,  she  had  started 
violently,  while  nursing,  when  a  paper  was  torn  some  eight  feet 
away  and  that  several  times  on  these  days  she  started  and  cried  out 
even  in  sleep,  when  a  paper  was  rustled  sharpie  as  her  father  sat  by 
the  bed.1 

In  the  first  week  she  did  not  seem  to  notice  when  on  his  return 
in  the  afternoon  her  father  sat  close  by,  reading  aloud  or  talking; 
but  in  the  second  and  third  weeks  she  always  became  restless  at 
this  time.  The  more  modulated  voices  of  women  who  were  in  the 
room  the  rest  of  the  time,  appeared  not  to  affect  her  at  all. 

The  sensitiveness  to  sound  seemed  variable;  for  on  the  23d 
day,  when  I  purposely  rustled  paper  near  the  baby,  it  produced 
no  clear  reaction.  On  this  same  day,  also,  I  struck  an  ordinary 
table  call-bell  several  times,  suddenly  and  sharply,  at  a  distance 
of  2  feet  from  her  head,  and  several  times  at  1  foot,  without  calling 
out  any  certain  sign  of  hearing.  Once  at  6  inches  and  once  at  3 
inches  the  stroke  of  the  bell  was  followed  by  a  sudden  and  distinct 
wink;  but  when  nursing  she  did  not  wink,  even  when  the  bell  was 
struck  suddenly  three  inches  from  her  ear.  A  large  dinner  bell  at 
3  feet  and  2  feet  produced  a  slight  quiver  of  the  lids ;  when  it  was 
struck  at  1  foot  she  stopped  nursing  and  threw  one  hand  out.  In 
these  experiments,  I  took  especial  pains  to  allow  a  sufficient  inter- 
val between  the  strokes  of  the  bell,  as  I  noticed  that  the  decline 


1  Mrs.  Eleanor  Sharp  gives  me  the  note  that  the  first  sign  of  hearing  observed 
in  her  boy  was  that  on  the  3d  or  4th  day  he  was  disturbed  by  the  rattling  of  a 
newspaper  his  father  was  reading. 

(107) 


N 


108  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

of  sensibility  was  very  marked  if  the  sounds  followed  at  all 
closely. 

The  27th  day,  she  showed  no  sign  of  hearing  single  notes  on  the 
piano,  from  the  highest  to  the  lowest.  Yet  she  started  at  a 
hand-clap  behind  her  head,  and  as  her  hair  was  unusually  thick, 
it  seems  scarcely  likely  that  she  felt  any  puff  of  air.  The  next  day 
she  noticed  chords;  but  of  her  attention  to  these  I  will  speak 
under  a  separate  head  below. 

The  37th  day,  the  servant  brought  in  her  bath,  and  set  it  down 
abruptly,  so  that  the  tin  handles  rattled.  The  baby,  King  half 
asleep  on  my  lap,  started  violently,  with  a  cry  so  loud  that  it  brought 
in  her  grandfather  from  two  rooms  away  to  see  what  was  wrong; 
she  put  up  her  lip  with  the  first  crying  grimace  she  had  ever  made, 
and  showed  the  effect  of  the  fright  in  a  disturbed  face  for  five  min- 
utes. 

Yet  throughout  the  first  two  months  there  were  also  many 
times  when  she  failed  to  pay  any  attention  to  sounds  quite  as 
striking  as  the  few  she  did  notice.  The  great  variation  in  sensi- 
bility struck  me  especially  in  the  second  month. 

From  early  in  the  third  month  (69th  day),  I  note  repeatedly 
that  she  was  diverted  more  or  less  when  being  dressed  or  fretful  for 
any  other  reason,  by  hearing  things  rattled.  The  83d  day,  she 
showed  no  attention  to  a  watch  at  her  ear. 

In  the  fifth  month,  134th  day,  she  was  startled  while  nursing  by 
a  piece  of  coal  dropped  on  the  hearth,  and  sat  up  and  cried  a  little, 
with  a  pitiful  expression.  Other  instances  of  attention  to  sound  in 
the  second  and  third  months  occur  below,  under  other  heads. 

In  the  seventh  month,  202d  day,  I  note  as  an  unusual  incident 
her  starting  when  I  called  her  suddenly ;  but  in  the  eighth  month 
she  invariably  winked  if  a  door  slammed,  hands  were  clapped,  or 
articles  rapped  together  near  her. 

Early  in  the  ninth  month,  249th  day,  she  stopped  play  and 
turned  to  look,  hearing  a  pencil  dropped  behind  her  on  a  matted 
floor,  several  feet.away;  a  few  minutes  later  she  turned  to  look  when 
a  horse  stamped,  some  forty  feet  outside,  the  door  and  window 
being  closed.  Again,  she  stopped  in  the  midst  of  taking  her  food 
when  an  engine  whistled,  a  mile  away.  About  two  weeks  later, 
265th  day,  she  turned  to  look  when  a  woodpecker  struck  a  tree,  some 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  109 

fifty  feet  away.  In  the  tenth  month  she  was  quick  to  look  and 
listen  at  the  note  of  a  bird,  the  whistle  of  an  engine  miles  away, 
etc.,  —  once,  on  the  285th  day,  she  turned  at  the  chirp  of  a  cricket. 
Her  recognition  and  discrimination  of  sounds,  which  was  good 
before  the  end  of  the  year,  showed  clear  hearing. 


2.  Direction. 

The  earliest  ability  to  direct  her  look,  except  when  it  was  drawn 
along  by  something  on  which  her  eyes  were  fixed,  was  when  guided 
by  the  continuous  sound  of  the  piano  close  by,  as  already  mentioned 
under  Sight.  From  the  fifth  to  the  seventh  week  when  listening 
she  had  stared  into  the  face  of  the  person  who  was  playing,  as  if 
she  associated  that  with  the  sound;  but  on  the  45th  day  she  turned 
once  and  looked  at  the  keys,  and  on  the  57th  began  to  watch  them 
as  the  source  of  the  sound. 

The  93d  day,  just  before  she  was  three  months  old,  I  first  saw 
her  look  for  the  source  of  any  other  sound.  Someone  sneezed  — 
not  especially  loud  — when  she  was  nursing;  she  stopped  suddenly, 
seemed  startled,  and  made  sounds  of  discomfort,  and  her  mother 
lifted  her  into  a  sitting  position  to  divert  her,  on  which  she  turned 
her  head  and  looked  with  every  appearance  of  intention  in  the 
direction  of  the  sound.  I  was  told,  however,  that  a  few  days 
before  she  had  lifted  her  head  at  the  sound  of  the  fire  snapping,  and 
looked  as  if  to  see  where  it  came  from.  From  this  time  she  would 
occasionally  turn  her  head  to  seek  the  source  of  sounds,  —  once  in 
the  twentieth  week,  at  the  humming  of  a  teakettle,  —  and  by  the 
second  six  months  it  was  common. 

It  seemed  to  me  from  the  first  that  she  guided  her  look  quite 
accurately  toward  the  sound.  The  incident  of  her  locating  me  when 
I  called  to  her  over  the  top  of  a  tall  screen  (165th  and  168th  days) 
has  been  given  under  the  head  of  Sight  (p.  23);  also  that  of  her 
difficulty  in  finding  the  source  of  our  voices  when  we  called  to  her 
from  a  second-story  window,  —  and  this  difficulty  appeared  even  in 
the  thirteenth  month  (386th  day)  when  her  position  was  a  little 
unfavorable,  off  at  one  side  of  the  window.  I  found  on  the  early 
occasion,  when  she  was  about  six  months  old,  that  a  sufficiently 


I IO  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

sharp  and  striking  sound,  as  the  rustling  of  paper,  would  draw  her 
eyes  up  to  the  window  when  our  voices  failed. 

At  this  age  she  was  under  ordinary  circumstances  quite  accu- 
rate in  turning  toward  a  voice,  but  when  two  persons  were  close 
together  and  one  called  her,  she  would  occasionally  look  at  the 
wrong  one. 

In  the  tenth  month,  I  thought  she  located  sounds  remarkably 
well:  e.g.,  the  293d  day,  a  horse  stamping  inside  a  stable  eight  or 
ten  rods  away;  a  man  moving  about  inside  a  neighboring  building. 
Earlier,  the  283d  day,  I  had  seen  her  turn  at  sound  of  an  approach- 
ing wagon  to  watch  for  its  appearance  around  the  curve  of  the 
drive,  about  a  hundred  feet  away  ;  but  this  was  probably  inference 
and  memory,  rather  than  correct  location  of  the  sound;  and  on  the 
286th  day  when  she  heard  someone  moving  about  in  the  room 
above,  she  looked  at  the  stair  door,  instead  of  upward. 

In  the  fourteenth  and  fifteenth  months,  trying  to  find  me  in  a 
dark  room,  she  would  run  directly  toward  the   sound  of  my  voice. 

In  the  nineteenth  month  (593d  day)  hearing  a  church  bell  ring 
out  unusually  clear,  a  mile  away,  she  ran  toward  the  sound,  crying, 
"Bell!  ring!"  and  began  to  seek  the  bell  in  seeming  surprise.  It 
seemed  to  me  noticeable  that  when  she  had  no  knowledge  whatever 
of  the  whereabouts  of  the  bell,  she  should  move  without  hesitation 
in  the  right  direction.  Again,  in  the  twenty-eighth  month,  the 
school  bell,  also  a  mile  away,  began  to  ring;  the  child  stopped 
playing  and  cried,  "  I  heard  de  'cool  bell!"  She  was  familiar  with 
the  sound  of  this  bell,  but  the  schoolhouse  was  not  visible,  and, 
though  she  had  often  passed  it,  it  was  by  a  roundabout  road,  which 
started  out  in  an  entirely  different  direction.  "  Where  was  it  ?  — 
Point,"  I  said.  She  pointed  accurately.  Yet  once  in  the  twentieth 
month,  hearing  an  electric  bell  that  was  fixed  in  the  ceiling  of  a 
corridor  where  she  was  walking,  she  ran  about  looking  for  it  in 
vain.  It  would  seem,  comparing  this  incident  with  her  difficulty  in 
finding  us  when  we  called  from  upper  windows,  that  a  sound  con- 
siderably above  her  was  harder  to  locate  than  one  on  the  same 
level. 


The  Development  of  a  Child.  1 1 1 


3.  Recognition  and  Discrimination  of   Sounds. 

The  first  sign  of  this  that  I  detected  was  in  the  fifth  month, 
137th  day.  I  was  somewhat  hoarse  with  a  cold,  and  when  I  spoke 
the  baby  looked  and  listened  in  a  way  that  I  thought  showed  a  sense 
of  something  unusual  about  my  voice.  At  this  time  I  often  read 
softly  to  her  mother  as  she  nursed  the  baby  and  sang  low  to  her 
meanwhile.  On  the  149th  day,  as  I  did  this,  the  baby  suddenly 
raised  her  head  and  gave  me  an  inquiring  look,  evidently  for  the 
first  time  distinguishing  our  voices  as  two  separate  sounds.  At  just 
five  months,  when  about  to  reach  for  an  object  that  interested  her, 
she  stopped  half  a  dozen  times  in  succession  just  as  she  had  leaned 
forward  and  put  out  her  hand,  to  look  over  her  shoulder  with  a 
grave  and  puzzled  expression  at  the  sound  of  her  father's  voice  in 
conversation  the  other  side  of  the  room.  From  the  sixth  month 
on,  the  deep  voice  of  the  family  doctor,  unlike  that  of  any  member 
of  the  family,  affected  her,  and  on  the  229th  day  I  felt  her  thrill  in 
my  arms  every  time  the  deep  note  vibrated  as  he  spoke  loudly;  she 
looked  and  listened  whenever  he  spoke. 

In  the  eighth  month,  231st  day,  she  stopped  play  and  listened 
intently,  hearing  her  uncle  sing,  two  rooms  away,  with  the  doors 
closed  between ;  her  manner  made  me  think  she  was  trying  to  rec- 
ognize the  voice.  In  the  ninth  month,  257th  day,  she  crawled  twice, 
I  was  told,  to  the  door,  hearing  on  the  other  side  the  voice  of  her 
grandfather,  who  was  then  her  especial  favorite.  By  the  end  of  the 
eleventh  month  there  seemed  no  longer  any  doubt  that  she  did 
definitely  recognize  the  voices  of  the  family,  even  in  other  rooms, 
for  she  would  stop  and  listen,  then  beg  to  go  to  them. 

The  consciousness  of  difference  in  verbal  sounds  appeared 
almost  as  soon  as  consciousness  of  difference  in  voices.  During 
the  fifth  month  her  mother  and  grandmother  said  to  her  a  great 
deal,  '"Pa-pa,"  hoping  to  hasten  her  understanding  of  the  word. 
On  the  149th  day,  they  told  me,  she  imitated  the  motion  of  the  lips, 
and  apparently  amused  by  the  feeling,  laughed,  and  thereafter 
laughed  whenever  she  heard  the  sounds.     This  was  repeated  for 


112  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

my  observation,  and  although  at  first  the  word  spoken  by  me  did 
no:  produce  the  same  effect,  I  tried  again  when  she  was  undressed 
and  very  happy,  and  altered  the  vowel  (saying  poo-poo,  pup-pil, 
etc.)  and  the  consonant  (ba-ba,  ba-ba,  etc.),  and  found  that  she 
invariably  laughed,  chuckling  aloud  with  gayety  at  the  broad  vow- 
els, —  this  some  twenty  times.  She  was  more  amused  when  looking 
at  the  lips, but  smiled  in  any  case.  The  same  sounds  pronounced  in 
ordinary  speech,  without  the  marked  emphasis,  did  not  amuse.  No 
other  consonants  with  the  same  vowels  interested  her ;  though  on 
the  theory  that  the  amusement  lay  in  the  visible  action  of  the  lips 
and  the  association  with  a  novel  feeling  there  I  tried  in  especially. 
After  the  first  day  it  made  no  difference  which  of  us  said  papa  to 
her,  different  though  our  voices  and  inflections  were,  —  she  always 
laughed;  151st  day,  when  just  five  months  old,  she  once  stopped 
in  the  middle  of  a  cry  to  laugh  at  it.  After  some  ten  days  the 
invariable  gayety  at  the  sound  faded,  and  by  the  163d  day  it  no 
longer  amused,  but  a  rapid  pop-pop-pop-pop,  at  which  she  had  before 
only  stared  in  wonder,  now  made  her  smile,  and  often  thereafter. 
No  effort  to  associate  these  sounds  with  her  father  had  the  slightest 
success. 

Late  in  the  sixth  month,  173d  day,  she  turned  so  often  when 
her  name  was  called  that  I  tried  to  find  if  it  was  only  the  calling 
tone  she  recognized,  and  called  her  in  the  same  tone  by  several 
others.  I  found  she  would  turn  only  for  "Ruth,"  or  nicknames 
containing  a  similar  vowel  sound,  such  as  "Toots."  These  were,  it  is 
true,  nicknames  by  which  she  was  often  called,  but  for  others  quite 
as  frequent,  e.  g.,  "  Baby,"  she  would  not  turn.  In  five  or  six  careful 
trials,  made  behind  her,  I  found  that  she  would  turn  and  look 
intelligently  into  my  face  whenever  called  with  the  long  vowel  (u  or 
00);  and  though  trials  in  the  next  two  days  were  imperfectly  suc- 
cessful, they  were  entirely  so  on  the  178th  day,  and  thereafter. 

Just  at  the  beginning  of  the  eighth  month  she  began  to  know 
the  names  of  the  family,  and  thereafter  understanding  of  words 
steadily  developed. 

In  the  ninth  month,  249th  day,  she  stopped  play  and  listened 
with  a  look  of  surprise  for  a  minute  or  more,  hearing  the  piano  in 
the  farther  corner  of  the  next  room;  then  seeming  to  recognize  the 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  113 

sound,  began  to  utter  cries  of  appeal,  desiring  to  get  at  it  and  pound. 
Throughout  the  thirty-seventh  week  she  showed  the  same  excite- 
ment when  one  took  up  a  toy  cat  and  mewed  behind  it  that  she  did 
at  sight  of  the  real  cats,  and  quite  possibly  recognized  the  sound. 
She  certainly  did  at  a  year  old  (367th  day),  and  laughed  aloud  with 
recognition,  when  someone,  amusing  her  with  very  clever  animal 
noises,  imitated  a  cat. 

In  the  second  year  she  recognized  too  many  sounds  to  enumer- 
ate. E.  g.,  in  the  sixteenth  month  she  knew  a  bee's  humming,  and 
would  say,  "Bee!"  on  hearing  it.  In  the  same  month  she  used  the 
word  "squeak"  of  a  squeaking  made  in  the  pipes  by  the  water  run- 
ning out  of  the  tub;  and  asked  on  the  507th  day  what  the  birds 
—  who  were  chirping,  not  warbling  —  said,  she  answered,  after 
listening,  "  Squeak."  She  knew  at  this  time  the  most  distant  sound 
of  cars  rumbling,  or  the  whistle,  and  would  cry,  "Choo-choo!" 
When  she  was  twenty  months  old,  she  showed  appreciation  of  the 
difference  between  a  near  and  distant  whistle,  observing,  "  Choo- 
choo!  Way  off!  Hear  choo-choo  more  !  "  (i,  e.,  I  wish  to  hear  it 
more.)  Thediscrimination  was  the  more  difficult  as  she  was  indoors, 
and  the  sound  came  through  walls.  In  the  next  month  or  two, 
"Ruth  hear  choo-choo,  way  off,"  was  not  an  infrequent  remark. 

By  the  twenty-second  month  she  recognized  all  ordinary  bird 
notes  as  by  "birdies"  in  general,  and  the  owl's  and  quail's  she  knew 
specifically.  While  camping  in  the  woods  she  heard  a  bluejay's  note, 
was  told  what  it  was,  and  —  I  was  told  —  always  recognized  it  after- 
ward, crying  "  Booday!"  "  Ruth  did  hear  big  bluejay  birdy,"  and 
"Ruth  hear  fishman  horn,"  were  among  sentences  noted  by  her 
mother  just  after  the  close  of  the  twenty-third  month. 

In  the  thirty-first  month  I  thought  she  showed  some  sense  of 
the  phonic  values  of  the  letters;  her  grandmother  had  told  her  the 
spellings  s-o,  n-o,  etc.,  and  without  much  teaching  she  would  go 
through  four  or  five  of  these,  naming  the  word  when  the  spelling 
was  given  her,  without  mistake.  This  exercise  was  not  followed 
up,  however,  and  she  was  well  on  in  the  first  half  of  the  fourth 
year  before  it  became  unmistakable  that  spelling  a  word  suggested 
its  sound  to  her. 

Many  other  notes  that  bear  on  this  topic,  in  the  second  and 
third  years  especially,  are  reserved  for  the  subject  of  Speech. 


114  University  of  California.  [Vol.  l 

A  few  notes  on  the  emotional  effect  of  sounds  (other  than 
musical)  may  most  fitly  be  placed  here. 

The  baby's  amusement  in  the  fifth  month  at  certain  verbal 
sounds  has  been  spoken  of  above.  In  the  sixth  month,  163d  day, 
she  laughed  upon  hearing  a  little  child  cry.  About  the  end  of  the 
sixth  month  and  beginning  of  the  seventh  she  was  tried  several 
times  with  a  harsh  tone;  but  instead  of  crying,  like  Preyer's  baby, 
she  would  first  stare,  then  laugh. 

Two  or  three  instances  of  fear  at  sudden  sounds  in  the  earliest 
months  have  been  given.  Her  sensitiveness  to  the  doctor's  deep 
voice  (see  above)  had  also  much  of  fear;  but  this  was  first  mani- 
fested when  she  heard  it  just  as  she  lost  sight  of  her  mother  in  a 
strange  house,  and  may  have  been  caused  more  by  this  association 
than  by  any  quality  in  the  sound  itself. 

Near  the  end  of  the  twenty-first  month  (633d  day)  her  father,  to 
amuse  her,  set  off  some  fireworks  of  the  kind  called  "  volcanoes," 
which  begin  with  a  display  of  light  and  end  with  a  rapid  popping. 
She  had  in  her  seventeenth  month  listened,  sitting  close  by,  to  the 
exploding  of  many  packs  of  firecrackers  on  Chinese  New  Year, 
and  had  shown  only  the  most  absorbed  and  pleased  interest;  but 
on  this  occasion  as  soon  as  the  popping  —  a  little  heavier  in  quality 
than  the  crackers  —  began,  she  urged,  "  No  more !  "  Asked  if  she 
liked  the  light,  she  said,  "  Yes."  —  "The  noise  ?  "  —  "No."  A  few 
days  later  her  father  tried  again,  with  more  marked  result:  when 
the  popping  began,  she  cried  in  distress,  almost  in  tears,  "  Papa 
make  no  more !  "  then,  "Papa  make  light;  papa  make  no  more!" 
till  he  desisted.  She  was  in  her  mother's  arms,  inside  the  room, 
watching  the  fireworks  through  a  window,  and  the  feeling  did  not 
seem  to  me  exactly  fear,  but  perhaps  a  mixture  of  fear  and  aversion. 
The  next  Fourth  of  July,  a  year  later,  she  showed  a  touch  of  it 
again,  but  it  was  overcome  without  much  trouble.  (Since  the  close 
pf  the  three  years  covered  by  these  notes,  at  the  age  of  three  years 
and  five  months,  she  looked  on  at  an  exhibition  of  trained  lions  with 
quiet  enjoyment,  —  even  when  the  great  beasts  growled  and  pressed 
up  to  the  bars  close  by,  —  until  several  pistol  shots  were  fired ;  she 
started  violently  at  each  one,  and  at  once  wished  to  go,  saying,  "  I 
don't  want  to  'tay  any  longer  in  places  where  dey  have  such  ugly 
noises."     I  could  not  but  be  reminded  by  this  repugnance,  or  dread, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  115 

of  the  cowing  effect  of  explosive  noises  on  the  beasts  themselves, 
and  on  most  animals.) 

The  day  she  was  twenty-five  months  old  she  said  with  strong 
disapproval,  "  I  don't  like  that!"  hearing  a  frog  croak.  I  noticed 
no  other  instance  of  dislike  to  that  particular  sound. 

In  the  thirty-sixth  month  she  several  times  seemed  to  find  it 
very  disagreeable  if  I,  in  telling  an  animal  story,  imitated  the  dog's 
whine,  the  squirrel's  chatter,  etc.;  she  would  put  her  hand  over  my 
mouth  and  say,  "  Don't !  "  But  she  would  do  the  same  thing 
sometimes  if  I  introduced  some  pantomimic  motion,  and  I  suspect 
dislike  of  the  sound  itself  had  little  to  do  with  her  feeling,  which 
may  have  been  a  sort  of  sense  of  uncanniness  in  my  making  sounds 
or  motions  outside  my  proper  character. 


4.  Music. 

The  27th  day,  as  I  have  said,  the  baby  showed  no  attention  to 
single  notes  on  the  piano ;  but  the  next  day  when  fretting  with 
hunger  she  became  silent  on  hearing  chords  struck;  and  early  in 
the  29th  day  (evening)  again  stopped  fretting,  and  lay  and  listened 
to  chords  with  apparent  attention  for  five  minutes.  Thereafter  for 
some  weeks  this  was  the  method  of  diverting  her  if  she  became 
hungry  before  the  time  of  nursing;  she  would  lie  quiet  listening 
with  a  look  of  satisfaction,  gazing  into  the  face  of  the  person  that 
held  her,  or  after  the  57th  day  watching  the  keys.  On  this  day, 
when  fretting  with  hunger,  she  was  diverted  and  kept  intent  for  ten 
minutes  on  lively  music,  after  slow  music  had  failed  to  divert,  and 
might  have  listened  still  longer  had  we  tried. 

During  the  third  month  the  interest  in  the  piano  declined,  and 
other  means  had  to  be  found  for  keeping  her  patient  if  she  became 
hungry  too  soon.  During  the  fourth  month  it  was  a  pleasure, 
though  of  less  relative  importance  among  her  interests  than  in  the 
second  month.  Just  before  the  end  of  the  fourth  month,  121st  day, 
her  uncle  began  to  strike  the  piano,  concealed  from  her  by  a  high 
chair  back.  She  leaned  from  my  lap,  and  looked  first  one  side  then 
the  other  of  the  chair  back,  with  quick,  eager  turns  of  her  head.  I 
took  her  to  his  side,  and  sat  down  with  her.  She  watched  his 
fingers  eagerly,    and  I  could   feel   her  start  and  thrill  every    time 


1 16  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i, 

he  struck  a  heavy  chord  or  accented  a  note  strongly.  He  began 
to  sing  in  strong  baritone ;  she  gazed  alternately  up  to  his  face 
and  down  at  his  hands,  throwing  her  head  back  to  look  up,  with 
eyes  expanded,  brows  raised,  and  jaw  dropped;  she  alternated  her 
gaze  about  every  five  seconds  during  the  singing  of  a  stanza. 

In  the  fifth  month  also  pleasure  in  music  was  habitual,  but  still 
declining  in  relative  importance,  as  her  interests  grew.  On  the  133d 
day,  e.  g.,  she  listened,  sometimes  making  movements  with  hands  or 
feet,  or  little  sounds,  sometimes  looking  up  earnestly  at  me,  or  smil- 
ing at  her  mother.  The  twentieth  week  she  listened  to  the  piano 
daily  with  quiet  pleasure,  preferring  lively  music. 

In  the  sixth  month  she  cared  little  for  the  piano,  especially  as 
compared  with  seizing,  and  if  brought  near  it,  wished  to  handle  the 
keys  herself.  At  twenty-four  weeks  old  she  again  stared  with  a 
comical  look  of  surprise  at  her  uncle  when  singing,  but  did  not 
listen  as  attentively  as  before;  she  moved  about  in  my  lap  and  tried 
to  put  her  fingers  on  the  keys,  and  after  one  song  she  tired  and 
looked  about  for  other  occupation. 

Late  in  the  fourth  month,  the  120th  and  121st  days,  her  body 
quivered  with  excitement  on  hearing  her  uncle- whistle;  but  in  the 
eighth  month,  228th  day,  she  showed  little  interest  in  his  shrillest 
whistles,  looking  up  with  open  mouth  each  time  he  changed  the 
key  and  time,  then  returning  to  her  play. 

During  the  second  half  year  she  would  hardly  listen  to  music, 
desiring  always  to  get  her  own  hands  on  the  keys,  from  which  she 
had  discovered  she  could  bring  sounds.  She  usually  greeted  the 
beginning  of  music  with  joy  ("with  a  yell  of  delight,"  I  note;  or 
"laughs  aloud  and  flourishes  arms")  but  soon  lost  interest  if  not 
allowed  to  reach  the  keys  herself.  Once  in  the  seventh  month 
(211th  day)  she  sat  and  listened  with  joyous  laughter  to  playing 
and  singing;  and  once  in  the  eleventh  month  (328th  day)  when  two 
young  ladies  sang  with  the  piano  she  listened,  laughing  aloud, 
clapping  her  hands,  and  creeping  excitedly  first  toward  one,  then 
the  other;  but  she  soon  crept  to  the  piano,  and  getting  to  her  feet 
tried  to  reach  the  keys  herself,  and  cared  no  longer  to  listen. 

In  the  second  and  third  years  there  was  no  noteworthy  increase 
in  her  pleasure  in  music.  She  would  listen  with  more  or  less  interest 
for  a  minute  or  two,  then  turn  to  something  else.     Once  near  the 


shinn.i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  117 

end  of  the  fourteenth  month  (421st  day)  she  seemed  dissatisfied 
with  her  own  results  in  drumming  on  the  piano,  and  twice  after  try- 
ing a  little  while  wished  someone  else  to  take  her  place;  we  thought 
this  might  be  the  beginning  of  some  musical  perception,  but  it  did 
not  prove  to  be.  In  the  sixteenth  month,  458th  day,  she  was  a 
good  deal  interested  in  a  banjo  on  the  cars,  but  probably  as  much 
in  seeing  as  in  hearing;  and  so  again  in  the  frequent  singing  and 
playing  of  a  guest,  an  accomplished  musician,  in  the  sixty-ninth 
week.  Her  interest  in  handling  the  piano  herself  declined  at  this 
time,  but  returned  in  the  seventeenth  month,  and  in  the  seventy- 
sjcond  week  she  was  deeply  pleased  when  allowed  to  do  it.  This 
interest  was,  at  least  from  the  eighteenth  month,  in  part  merely 
imitative;  for  from  that  time  she  insisted  on  having  a  book  open  on 
the  rack,  and  would  at  intervals  turn  the  leaves  over. 

She  must  have  recognized  in  a  general  way  the  sound  of  music, 
for  the  word,  picked  up  in  the  seventeenth  month  with  enthusiasm 
and  used  ("moo")  in  begging  to  be  allowed  to  strike  the  piano,  was 
generalized  correctly  once  in  the  twentieth  month  (598th  day)  when 
she  went  to  listen  to  musicians  on  the  ferry  boat,  and  commented, 
"Play  music!"  though  their  instruments  bore  no  resemblance  to 
the  piano. 

Her  mouth  organ  was  better  liked  than  whistles  throughout  the 
whole  second  year,  and  she  took  at  intervals  real  pleasure  in  it. 
Once  when  just  twenty-one  months  old,  she  was  diverted  from 
desire  to  go  outdoors  —  always  a  persistent  desire  —  by  it,  after 
other  things  had  failed. 

In  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  once  asked  me  to  play  for  her, 
and  was  dissatisfied  when  I  struck  a  few  chords  and  scales,  urging 
me  to  "play."  —  "  Do  you  mean  as  mamma  does?  " — "  Yes."  When 
I  told  her  I  could  not,  she  gave  it  up  at  once.  This  desire  to  hear 
real  playing  was  quite  exceptional.  Her  own  performance  was 
usually  perfectly  satisfactory  to  her,  and  I  note  over  and  over  that 
she  did  not  care  to  listen  to  playing.  She  did  not  thump  roughly, 
however,  but  from  the  twenty-ninth  month,  if  not  earlier,  touched 
the  piano  softly,  with  a  good  imitation  of  her  mother's  manner. 

Her  mother  had  always  sung  her  lullabies,  and  it  is  probable  she 
had  taken  some  pleasure  in  them;  but  beyond  going  to  sleep  under 
the  combined  influence  of  these  and  a  rocking  chair,  she  did  not 


1 1 8  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

show  any  such  pleasure  till  the  twenty-first  month,  when  she  was 
laid  in  her  own  bed  and  sung  to  sleep  there,  and  would  now  and 
then  ask  her  mother  to  sing.  As  early  as  the  sixteenth  month, 
she  sometimes  tried  to  get  her  uncle  to  sing  or  whistle,  —  doubtless 
appreciating  the  more  lusty  volume  of  his  performance. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  eighteenth  month  she  liked  to  touch 
individual  notes  and  listen  to  the  dying  of  the  sound,  instead  of 
merely  thumping;  and  also  seemed  for  the  first  time  to  get  some 
idea  of  difference  in  pitch,  for  she  would  reach  in  one  direction  and 
the  other  along  the  keyboard  to  produce  high  and  low  tones.  Late 
in  the  twenty-first  month  (633d  day)  she  commented  on  a  difference 
in  pitch:  she  was  in  the  habit  of  listening  with  interest  to  a  squeak- 
ing in  the  pipes  as  the  water  ran  out  of  the  bathtub,  and  on  this 
occasion  when  the  sound  for  some  reason  rose  a  little  in  pitch,  she 
cried  with  profound  emphasis  and  enthusiasm,  "Ain't  that  cunning 
little  baby  squeak ! "  A  month  later,  the  665th  day,  at  the  piano,  she 
reached  far  up  the  keyboard  and  struck  notes  as  high  as  she  could 
touch,  calling  them  "little."  In  the  twenty-third  month,  when  she 
had  once  asked  me  to  join  her  in  a  monotonous  chant  of  "  Ba-boo, 
ba-boo,  ba-boo,"  that  she  was  amusing  herself  with,  she  was  annoyed 
when  I  raised  the  pitch  considerably,  and  commanded,  "  Don't, 
aunty,  say  ba-boo  that  way."  She  did  not  seem  to  mind  how  much 
I  lowered  it. 

Attempts  to  have  her  recognize  any  air  failed  completely  till  the 
second  quarter  of  the  third  year.  From  the  twentieth  month,  if 
asked  "What  is  it?"  of  any  air,  however  familiar,  she  would  say, 
"  See-saw,"  or,  "  See-saw,  Ma'  Daw: "  until  late  in  the  twenty-second 
month,  ivhenshe  once  hit  correctly  another,  — by  accident  probably, 
for  thereafter  for  days,  elated  by  congratulations,  she  was  disposed 
to  recognize  all  tunes  as  "Little  Brown  Thrush."  She  was  tried 
not  only  with  airs  on  the  piano,  with  and  without  chords,  but  with 
the  voice;  and  humming,  or  the  use  of  distinct  syllables  that  might 
confuse,  was  avoided.  At  last,  in  the  middle  of  the  twenty-eighth 
month,  she  recognized  when  thus  sung  without  words,  "I  went  to 
the  animal  fair,"  a  popular  round,  in  which  the  time  is  very  marked. 
Four  days  later,  she  knew  "John  Brown  had  a  little  Injun,"  when 
picked  out  without  chords  on  the  piano.     She  asked  for  this  by 


shinn-I  The  Development  of  a  Child.  [ig 

name  several  times  about  the  same  date.  Both  seemed  unmistaka- 
ble recognitions,  yet  I  know  of  no  other  within  the  third  year,  —  if 
I  except  an  apparent  one  earlier  than  either,  in  the  twenty-fifth 
month,  when  just  after  "Lauriger  Horatius,"  she  began  to  chant  a 
fragment  from  "John  Morgan,"  which  she  had  heard  sung  to  the 
air  of  Lauriger. 

(In  the  first  half  of  the  fourth  year  she  has  learned  to  recognize 
half  a  dozen  melodies  without  hesitation.) 

Though  it  was  doubtless  by  their  time  that  she  first  recognized 
airs,  her  sense  of  rhythm  was  certainly  very  little  developed.  In 
the  earliest  months,  rhythmical  motion  must  have  had  the  usual 
effect  of  putting  her  to  sleep;  but  a  rough  motion, —  a  joggling,  — 
even  when  not  very  regular,  was  more  effective  than  a  smooth, 
swinging  one.  In  the  seventh  week  I  noted  that  she  did  not  go  to 
sleep  in  her  carriage  while  it  was  drawn  to  and  fro  on  the  paved 
veranda,  but  that  even  if  fretting,  she  would  stop  as  soon  as  the 
wheels  struck  the  gravel  walk,  and  lie  still  and  placid  with  open 
eyes  till  they  closed  with  sleep.  She  usually,  but  not  always,  from 
the  earliest  months  on,  became  sleepy  when  driving,  but  not  so 
frequently  on  a  train  or  street  car;  the  effect  of  the  open  air  is  of 
course  to  be  taken  into  account  here.  She  was  usually  after  the 
eleventh  week  put  to  sleep  with  a  rocking  chair  and  singing  (aided 
for  months  by  a  rubber  nipple),  but  this  was  not  so  easy  or  sure  a 
way  as  to  take  her  driving;  and  for  some  time  in  the  fourth  month 
it  was  necessary  to  begin  by  setting  her  upright  on  the  point  of  the 
knee  and  trotting  her  roughly  and  monotonously,  while  crooning 
"John  Brown  had  a  little  Injun,"  till  she  grew  sleepy;  then  her 
mother  could  cuddle  her  up  in  her  arms  and  resort  to  less  emphatic 
rhythms. 

I  never  at  any  time  in  the  first  three  years  saw  any  tendency  to 
fall  into  rhythm  with  any  motion  of  hands,  feet,  or  body;  but  in  the 
sixteenth  month  (478th  day)  when  someone  caught  the  child  up 
and  danced  with  her  in  arms,  she  showed  delight  that  distinctly 
exceeded  what  she  showed  over  being  simply  jumped  about  with, 
and  she  conceived  an  unusual  affection  for  the  guest  who  did  it. 

When  she  was  twenty-eight  months  old,  I  showed  her  how  to 
swing  the  kindergarten  balls,  saying,  "High  —  low,"  or  "Far  — 


1 20  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

near,"  in  time  with  the  motions.  She  was  pleased,  and  wished  to 
do  it,  and  imitated  the  action  with  other  objects  and  with  her  arm, 
but  could  not  be  taught  to  do  it  in  any  rhythm,  or  to  say  the  words 
in  time  with  the  motion ;  and  although  she  liked  the  exercise  and 
occasionally  performed  it  spontaneously,  it  was  not  till  the  second 
week  of  the  thirtieth  month  that  she  learned  to  do  it  in  time. 

Efforts  to  teach  her  to  march,  whether  in  time  to  music  or  keep- 
ing step  with  us,  were  futile.  Once  on  the  665th  day,  in  especial 
hilarity  at  being  allowed  on  a  very  warm  evening  to  scamper  about 
naked,  she  would  from  time  to  time  stop  and  sit  down  in  her  little 
rocking  chair  and  rock  vigorously,  shouting,  aR<1-/n>,  Too-boo!" 
("Rockaby,  Tootyboo,"  —  a  favorite  nickname),  stamping  with  her 
feet  completely  out  of  time  with  her  voice. 

Unlike  other  children  I  have  known,  she  showed  no  disposition 
to  pick  up  the  words  of  little  jingles,  which  usually  attract  by 
their  rhythm  ;  and  some  efforts  made  to  teach  them  early  in  the 
third  year  were  resisted.  Near  the  end  of  the  twenty-sixth  month, 
however,  she  learned  to  repeat  four  lines  of  "  Little  Drops  of 
Water"  without  much  prompting;  but  it  was  more  than  two 
months  later  before  she  first  repeated  them  correctly,  and  during 
the  interval  she  steadily  refused  really  to  learn  any  other  lines, 
though  she  would  pick  up  snatches  of  a  few  words  here  and  there. 
In  the  latter  part  of  the  twenty-ninth  month  and  during  the  thirtieth 
she  was  not  unwilling  to  learn  jingles,  and  learned  half-a-dozen  or 
more,  more  or  less  spontaneously;  she  would  occasionally  repeat 
these,  or  pretend  to  sing  them  at  the  piano.  At  this  time,  also,  she 
seemed  pleased  in  listening  to  rhymes,  which  she  had  not  cared  to 
do  for  months :  in  the  first  week  of  the  thirtieth  month  she  listened 
carefully  to  "The  Owl  and  the  Pussy-cat,"  and  said  at  the  end  of 
each  stanza,  "  I  like  that  story."  She  usually,  however,  refused  to 
say  the  words  of  the  little  action  songs  her  mother  tried  to  teach 
her,  though  she  liked  accompanying  them  with  the  motions  if  her 
mother  would  attend  to  the  words.  After  the  thirtieth  month  she 
became  averse  again  to  repeating  rhymes,  soon  forgot  them  all,  and 
did  not  recover  them. 

'  The  rhythm  of  these  verses  seemed  to  give  her  no  help  in 
remembering  them  ;  and  so  far  from  preserving  metre  even  if  she 
marred  the  sense,  as  some  children  do,  she  would  constantly  hold 


shinn.]  The- Development  of  a  Child.  121 

to  her  memory  of  the  meaning,  at  any  cost  to  the  metre.  Thus  in 
"The  Barberry  Bush,"  for  "This  is  the  way  we  go  to  sleep,"  she 
said,  "This  is  the  way  we  lie  down  and  go  to  sleep;"  and  in  like 
manner,  "One,  two,  buckle  up  my  shoe,"  and,  "Rockaby,  baby,  on 
the  tree  top;  when  the  wind  blows  the  cradle  will  tip  over"  were 
gratuitous  dislocations  of  rhythm. 

"Here  is  the  beehive,  but  where  are  the  bees? 
Hidden  away  where  nobody  sees; 
Soon  they  '11  come  creeping  out  of  the  hive,  — 
One,  —  two,  — three,  — four,  —  five," 


was  rendered, 


"He'  i'  bee-hi',  bu' fa  bee? 
Hi'  'way,  fa  nobo'  see. 
One,  two,  free,  fo',  fi',  come  out," 

and  Jack  Horner  became 

"Li-tel  Ja'  Ho'-nay 
Sa'  in  co'-nay, 
A'  pull  ou'  p'um,  a'  sai',  '  Fa  g'ea'   bo'  am  I! '  " 

In  the  following,  recited  upon  seeing  a  picture  of  Bopeep,  appears 
still  more  indifference  to  rhyme  than  rhythm:  — 

"She  has  lost  her  little  baa-lambies, 

And  don't  know  where  to  find  them. 
So  leave  them  alone  and  they  '11  come  back, 
And  bring  their  tails  behind  them." 

Even  when  she  repeated  verses  fairly  correctly,  her  frequent  omis- 
sion of  articles  and  her  disposition  to  dock  syllables  jarred  the 
metre  more  or  less,  and  she  never  made  the  instinctive  effort  to 
correct  this  by  extra  syllables  that  children  sometimes  do.  (Since 
the  close  of  the  third  year,  I  have  seen  some  signs  of  appreciation 
of  the  metrical  movement  of  verse.) 

No  imitation  of  singing  showed  the  faintest  attempt  to  repro- 
duce the  time  of  the  original  (unless  in  the  one  instance  of  four  or 
five  syllables  of  "See-saw,  Marjory  Daw,"  in  the  twenty-second 
month)  until  the  thirtieth  month,  when  the  time  of  "John  Brown 
had  a  Little  Injun"  was  fairly  well  rendered.  Her  dislike  to  try 
to  imitate  any  known  songs  after  this  made  it  impossible  to  watch 
progress  in  catching  their  rhythm,  but  in  occasional  snatches  such 

progress  was  visible. 
9 


122  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

As  early  as  the  sixteenth  month  repeated  efforts  were  made  to 
have  the  child  take  a  note,  but  she  proved  utterly  incapable  of  even 
understanding  what  was  wanted,  and  would  simply  shout  back  the 
syllable  used,  without  an  effort  at  pitch,  or  even  musical  tone.  By 
the  twenty-sixth  month,  if  a  note  was  given  followed  by  one  an 
octave  higher,  she  would  so  far  imitate  as  to  give  back  two  syllables 
of  which  the  second  was  the  higher,  but  not  even  in  pure  musical 
tone;  nor  could  she  follow  a  downward  change  of  pitch  at  all. 
Thinking  that  a  connected  bit  of  melody,  with  the  words,  might  be 
easier  to  repeat  than  single  notes,  her  mother  tried  at  the  end  of 
this  month  to  get  some  imitation  of  "Little  drops  of  water,"  but  in 
vain;  then  to  train  the  child  to  give  back  the  two  notes  at  the  end  of 
the  line,  but  although  twice  she  did  approach  them  she  oftener 
repeated  wildly,  either  carrying  the  higher  note  up  to  falsetto,  or 
reversing  high  and  low,  or  beginning  far  below  pitch  in  order  to 
emphasize  the  rise.  By  the  thirtieth  month  she  understood  the 
directions  "Higher,"  and  "  Lower,"  and  did  several  times  seem  to 
approximate  a  note;  but  it  is  likely  this  was  accidental,  and  she 
made  no  farther  progress  in  the  third  year.  (Within  the  first  half 
of  the  fourth,  since  the  close  of  this  record,  she  is  more  willing  to 
try,  can  guide  her  voice  better,  does  not  go  so  far  wrong,  and  now 
and  then  even  takes  a  note  correctly.) 

When  her  first  efforts  at  imitating  singing  were  made,  she  did 
not  seem  even  to  have  perceived  any  difference  in  tone  between 
singing  and  speech.  In  the  seventeenth  month  she  began  to  imitate 
both  reading  aloud  and  singing  by  the  same  sound,  —  a  monotonous 
la-la-la,  with  scarcely  even  a  chanting  tone,  and  it  was  weeks  before 
she  made  any  differentiation  in  it  for  singing.  Once  in  the  twentieth 
month,  hearing  the  name  of  a  guest  who  had  sung  a  great  deal 
while  in  the  house  in  the  sixty-ninth  week,  she  began  to  say, 
"  Loo-loo-la,"  over  and  over  in  a  loud  tone.  Urged,  "What  does 
Ruth  mean?"  she  finally  said  with  an  air  of  slight  offense,  "Si.' 
Izha," —  sing  like  Ivy.  (This  was  in  the  eighty-fourth  week ;  and  the 
length  of  the  memory  is  certainly  surprising:  it  was  not  purely  a 
sound  memory,  however,  for  she  had  taken  much  interest  in  seeing 
this  lady  sing,  and  had  had  a  great  fancy  for  her,  and  talked  of  her 
for  some  clays  after  she  was  gone;  at  least  once  in  the  interval,  also, 
in  the  seventy-seventh  week,  the  memory  had  been  brought  up.) 


sihnn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  123 

"Loo-loo-la"  was  probably  a  reminiscence  of"  Hallelujah,"  which  she 
often  heard  her  grandfather  sing;  and  in  the  twenty-second  month 
when  asked  what  grandpa  sang,  she  would  chant,  " Ld-loo-ld,"  or 
similar  syllables,  instead  of  trying  to  give  any  reminiscence  of  the 
tune.  In  like  manner,  other  songs  were  remembered  by  the  words 
and  correctly  attributed  to  the  people  who  sang  them,  and  this  even 
in  a  case  where  the  words  were  probably  unintelligible  (viz.,  "Where's 
your  mule,  O  where'  s  your  mule?"  in  John  Morgan;  rendering  this, 
'•  Fa-moo-lo!  Fa-moo-lo!"  she  knew  the  song  by  it.)  She  never  at 
any  time  tried  to  reproduce  an  air,  except  in  the  one  instance  of 
''See-saw,  Marjory  Daw"  (mentioned  above  in  connection  with 
rhythm) ;  in  repeating  these  words  she  made  a  faint  attempt  at 
imitation  in  the  opening  syllables.  At  the  end  of  the  twenty-sixth 
month,  when  she  had  become  fairly  familiar  with  the  words  of 
"  Little  Drops  of  Water,"  her  mother  persuaded  her  to  undertake  to 
sing  it  through  independently;  and  for  about  three  weeks  she  could 
occasionally  be  got  to  do  this.  The  words  were  merely  shouted 
out  in  a  loud,  chanting  drawl  at  a  medium  pitch,  with  occasionally 
a  syllable  in  high  falsetto,  —  a  ridiculous  burlesque.  She  was  quite 
serious  about  it,  but  did  not  like  much  to  do  it,  and  seemed  to  be 
aware  it  was  not  right,  and  to  feel  a  little  sheepish  about  it;  and 
after  a  few  weeks  she  refused  to  continue  it. 

Once  in  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  was  taken  into  Sunday 
school,  where  she  tried  to  sing  loudly  with  the  children,  without 
any  reference  to  the  tune,  to  most  confusing  result,  as  may  be 
imagined. 

In  my  notes  on  rhythm  I  necessarily  have  gone  outside  the  sub- 
ject of  Hearing,  since  rhythm  is  not  perceived  by  hearing  alone. 
In  order  to  complete  my  notes  on  Music  by  those  on  the  spontane- 
ous development  of  singing,  —  which  was  quite  a  different  thing 
from  the  futile  efforts  at  imitation  of  our  songs,  — ■  .1  can  scarcely 
omit  some  on  the  improvisation  of  words,  although  it  takes  me  still 
farther  outside  the  subject. 

The  first  sign  of  any  disposition  in  the  baby  to  express  herself 
musically  was  brought  out  by  the  hearing  of  music:  this  was  on 
the  fifty-ninth  day,  when  the  mother  began  to  play,  and  the  baby  at 
once  set  up  a  soft  singing  murmur  in  monosyllables,  u,  it,  and  kept 


1 24  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

it  up  as  long  as  the  music  lasted.  This  I  did  not  hear  myself,  and 
could  not  get  her  to  repeat  in  so  marked  a  degree:  she  would  utter 
the  murmur,  but  did  not  keep  it  up.  Soon  after,  she  fell  into  the 
habit  of  noticing  the  beginning  only  of  music  with  one  or  two 
syllables  of  the  murmur:  this  habit  lasted  till  sometime  in  the  fourth 
month.  The  eighty-third  day,  a  strong,  heavy  chord  on  the  piano 
brought  this  coo  from  across  the  room;  she  had  always  before  been 
close  by. 

At  eight  months  old  (243d  day)  on  hearing  chorus  singing,  she 
joined  in  with  a  disconcerting  squeal  on  every  high  note;  and  dur- 
ing the  ninth  month  had  a  note  of  her  own,  which  she  sometimes, 
but  not  always,  contributed  on  hearing  singing,  without  attempt  at 
concord.  Except  a  crooning  while  falling  asleep,  at  a  year  old,  this 
was  the  extent  of  disposition  to  sing  during  the  first  year. 

In  the  second  year,  she  showed  none  until  after,  in  the  seven- 
teenth month,  she  had  begun  to  try  to  imitate  singing,  as  mentioned 
above,  —  possibly  led  thereto  by  the  efforts  to  get  her  to  take  a  note 
then,  in  the  eighteenth  month,  she  took  up  certain  prattles,  or 
warbles,  with  which  she  amused  herself,  laughing  aloud  at  the 
sounds  and  trying  different  tones.  They  were  more  like  the  bab- 
bling before  she  learned  to  speak  than  like  any  real  musical  attempt, 
but  from  this  time,  a  sort  of  tuneless  crooning  as  she  was  about  her 
play  walking,  or  driving,  became  a  regular  expression  of  happiness 
Occasionally  snatches  from  songs  entered  into  it, —  as" 'Loo-loo-loo-la ,' 
or  "Fa-moo-lo,"  mentioned  above;  but  usually  it  was  in  meaningless 
syllables,  one  succession  of  these  favored  for  a  time,  then  another, 
—  " Ba-ba-ba-ba-ba,"  in  the  twenty-first  month,  "La-la-la-la-la," 
and  "Ba-boo,  ba-boo,  ba-boo,"  in  the  twenty-second,  changing  in  the 
twenty-third  to  "Boo-ba,  boo-ba,"  then  "L%-l%-lcc,  1%-1%-lcc;  "  etc.  She 
took  extreme  satisfaction  in  these  exercises,  and  once  (662d  day) 
when  some  one  of  the  others  in  the  carriage  with  her  would  begin 
to  sing,  she  would  command,  "Don't  sing!"  and  strike  up  herself 
with  her  loud  tuneless  syllables.  She  regarded  this  as  singing, 
and  if  asked  what  she  was  doing  would  say,  "Sing."  Once,  late  in 
the  twenty-third  month,  hearing  someone  say,  "Let  us  go  try  that 
song,"  she  began  to  chant  and  croon  in  her  fashion.  Yet  she  also 
sometimes  used  such  expressions  as  "Say  ba-boo,"  not  "Sing,"  and 
evidently  had  little  idea  of  any  difference  between  speech  and  sing- 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  125 

About  the  end  of  the  second  year,  the  pitch  in  these  chantings, 
which  had  been  scarcely  varied  from  a  monotone,  began  to  take 
wider  range ;  but  for  nearly  three  months  of  the  third  year  it  was 
still  hardly  distinguishable  from  the  imitation  of  reading  aloud,  of 
which  also  the  child  was  very  fond  in  this  quarter-year.  The  con- 
trast between  her  joy  in  her  own  "singing"  as  she  ran  anil  frisked 
about,  and  her  reluctant  efforts  to  sing  "Little  Drops  of  Water," 
was  striking:  and  it  is  also  noticeable  that  in  the  spontaneous  utter- 
ances she  did  not  vary  the  loud,  monotonous  chant  with  the  gro- 
tesque drawls  and  falsetto  syllables  used  in  the  other  case;  and  this 
was  true  whether  she  was  chanting  fragments  of  words  from  songs, 
or  as  was  more  frequent,  using  a  mere  jargon.  The  syllables  used 
(both  in  imitating  reading  and  in  "singing")  had  more  variety;  e.g. 
" A-hCi-b%-azv-'i.v%.     A-w\-a-h<14n-a-iSi-i<-h<\-zv%." 

Late  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  the  chanting  grew  perceptibly 
more  rhythmic,  varied,  and  began  for  the  first  time  to  take  a  sing- 
ing tone;  while  the  syllables  increased  in  variety  so  strikingly  that 
I  began  to  suspect  she  was  really  using  words,  which  I  failed  to 
catch  till  one  evening  in  the  middle  of  the  twenty-sixth  month  I 
heard  her  croon,  lying  on  the  floor,  "Go  to  sleep,  Ruth.  Go  to 
sleep,  little  baby  Ruth.     Go  to  sleep,  dear  little  baby  Ruth." 

The  next  morning  I  allowed  her  to  pile  up  a  number  of  books 
on  the  lounge,  and  sat  taking  notes  as  she  prattled  to  me  about 
what  she  was  doing.  By  and  by  she  began  to  turn  her  comments 
into  a  sort  of  rhythm:  "Mi'-ce'  [Milicent]  I  going  to  get  more 
book,  book,  book.  .  .  .  Ruth,  you  know,  I  going  to  get  more 
book,  book,  book."  "  Mi'-ce',  I  going  to  build  nice,  nice,  nice 
house."  "I  going  get  more  book,  Mi'-ce',  Mi'-ce',  Mi'-ce',"  — 
then  passed  into  a  jargon  in  a  singing  tone,  ''  E-dd-ive-ve.  Sd-fd- 
le-ve-tue-de,"  and  went  to  the  piano  to  continue  while  touching 
the  keys.  On  the  same  day,  having  heard,  "  I  went  to  the  animal 
fair,''  lustily  sung,  she  chanted  snatches  from  it  while  about  her 
play,  and  within  the  week  I  heard  her  chanting  over  and  over, 
"Here  we  go  round  the  barberry  bush;"  but  for  the  most  part  mere 
jargon,  as  "  Da-la-wa-zve"  was  preferred. 

By  another  month  the  imitation  of  reading  and  the  "singing" 
were  distinctly  differentiated,  the  one  keeping  the  unmodulated 
tone,  differing  from  speech  only  in  loudness,  monotony,  and  expres- 


126  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

sionlessness,  while  the  singing  was  now  really  modulated  and  had 
intervals  that  gave  it  a  musical  sound  without  any  recognizable 
melody.  Once  in  the  twenty-sixth  month  I  heard  the  child  singing 
the  numbers  (which  she  knew  up  to  ten)  quite  rhythmically. 

During  the  second  quarter  of  the  year,  one  or  two  odd  efforts  at 
improvisation  occurred.  In  the  first  week  of  the  twenty-ninth  month, 
she  was  heard  chanting  something  to  herself  as  her  mother  picked 
her  up  to  take  her  to  bed.  We  could  not  make  out  the  words  for 
some  seconds,  and  when  we  did,  she  was  chanting :  "  The  gull  birdy 
has  caught  a  rock.  —  The  gull  birdy  has  caught  a  bird.  —  The  gull 
birdy  has  caught  a  cracker."  Finding  that  we  were  interested,  she 
became  somewhat  proud  of  the  song,  and  said  she  wanted  to  go  to 
the  piano  and  "sing  about  a  kitty;  "  and  touching  the  keys,  chanted 
"  The  kitty  has  caught  a  book,"  etc.,  putting  in  the  names  of  various 
articles,  as  her  eyes  fell  on  them.  The  original  suggestion  must 
have  been  from  the  gulls  she  had  seen  about  the  ferry-boat  seizing 
bread  and  crackers  thrown  to  them;  and  could  we  have  heard  the 
beginning  of  the  improvisation  we  might  have  found  an  intelligible 
theme,  on  which  she  varied  at  random.  For  more  than  a  week  she 
was  fond  of  singing  to  herself,  "  The  gull  birdy  has  caught  a  rock," 
then  fell  back  on  the  jargon  of  syllables. 

During  the  rest  of  the  quarter  she  sometimes  varied  her  prattle 
with  chanted  fragments  of  words  that  seemed  to  have  stuck  in  her 
memory  by  their  rhythm:  thus  in  the  last  week  of  the  thirtieth 
month  she  interpolated  amid  broken  comments  on  what  she  was 
doing,  "  Mrs.  —  Mrs.  —  Mrs.  —  and  Pifesso'  —  Mrs.  —  Mrs.  — 
B'ozvn."  Several  times  in  this  month  she  spontaneously  broke  out 
with  some  of  the  songs  she  had  learned,  once  sitting  down  at  the 
piano  and  going  through  her  whole  list,  and  once,  with  some  help, 
carrying  through  "The  Barberry  Bush,"  with  all  the  motions,  fairly 
well;  but  on  the  whole  she  was  averse  to  any  attempt  to  regulate 
her  voice  and  preferred  to  shout,  caper,  and  squeal,  sometimes 
dancing  about  a  little  and  singing  in  her  own  fashion,  rather  than 
to  use  words  of  any  sort. 

In  the  remaining  half-year  I  noted  no  singing  except  of  this 
unordered  sort,  till  the  last  fortnight  of  the  third  year,  when  after 
hearing  " Trancadillo,"  she  went  about  all  day  caroling  not  unmu- 
sically   though    without    tune,  "  T'ancadillo,  O   T'aucadillo"    soon 


shins.;  The  Development  of  a   Cliild.  \2J 

changed  to  "Cazade-zoo,  0  Cazade-zvo;"  and  this  she  carried  on  into 
a  medley  of  words  and  wordless  syllables,  e.  g.,  "Cazade-wo,  0 
Cazade-zvo,  0  sect  la-da-zva-adewo,  an'  de  s'eetosiat  [sweet ocean]." 
(Cazadero  is  the  name  of  a  place  at  which  she  had  been.)  She  danced 
about  joyously  as  she  uttered  these  strings  of  words.  The  next  day 
she  came  to  me  and  said  earnestly,  "I  know  a  bird  song."  I  asked 
her  to  sing  it,  and  she  began  to  twitter  syllables  in  a  high  tone,  skip- 
ping about  and  waving  her  little  arms,  mixing  meaningless  syllables 
with  fragmentary  words  about,  "  Come,  birdy,  — ■  birdy  come."  She 
repeated  this  several  times  in  the  day,  and  it  became  evident  that 
the  song  and  dance  involved  going  away  to  a  distance  and  coming 
back.  Her  mother  made  out  the  words,  "Little  birdy  fly"  —  then 
after  some  jargon,  "  Come  home  to  the  nest."  Such  an  invention  was 
entirely  out  of  keeping  with  her  ways,  and  we  thought  it  impossible 
that  it  was  her  own;  but  except  that  her  mother  remembered  trying 
to  teach  her  such  a  song  more  than  a  year  before,  we  could  learn  no 
source  for  it,  and  she  herself  insisted  in  evident  good  faith  that 
mamma  had  not  told  her,  no  one  had  told  her, — -  she  had  told  it  to 
herself.  If  it  was  original,  it  was  exceptional,  imitative,  and  not 
characteristic. 

She  had  now  a  great  ambition  to  sing,  but  utterly  rejected  all 
known  songs,  refusing  order  or  sense.  Fragments  of  words  of  her 
own,  mixed  with  syllables,  as  "la,  la,"  made  up  the  songs,  —  about 
which  she  was  grave  and  earnest.  "I  know  a  nice,  pitty  song, 
au'tee;  I  can  sing  it  on  de  pee-anny,"  she  said,  looking  very  ear- 
nestly into  my  face.  She  took  me  to  the  piano,  and  touching  the 
keys  at  random  chirped  in  falsetto,  without  tune,  but  marking  the 
rhythm  somewhat :  — 

"All  about  de  pitty  birdies, 

La-la-la,  de  birdies,  de  birdies, 

All  about  de  pitty  birdies,  pitty,  pitty  birdies. 

"  Dat  's  de  song.  I  know  anuvvay  pitty  song.  Dis  is  about 
books: — 

"All  about  de  pitty  books, 

De  wed  books,  de  b'ack  books,  de  books,  de  la-la. 

Dat  song  is  about  a  b'ack  book;  b'ack  all  ovay;  b'ack  inside  and 

back  outsjde.  — 

"All  about  de  U'ke/l  Joe,  — 
d'kell  Joe,  a' d  Au'tee,  a'd  Roof  IVelmo'  S'inn,  de  names, 
De  b'ack  name,  defile  names,  de  wed  names,  all  about  de  names" 


128  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

This  was  suggested  by  her  uncle's  coming  in  and  speaking  to 
her.  She  went  on,  chanting  other  words  in  like  manner,  from 
random  suggestions. 

About  a  week  later,  in  the  highest  spirits,  racing  about,  shout- 
ing and  singing,  she  went  once  to  the  piano,  and  sat  many  minutes 
touching  the  keys  and  warbling  at  the  top  of  her  voice,  without 
tune,  but  with  a  joyous  birdlike  musical  effect.  I  took  down  the 
words  of  one  song:  — 

"  All  about  de  circus  coming, 
Coming  to  Centervillc,  to  Niles, 
To  all  de  people '  s  houses." 

I  may  have  exaggerated  her  marking  of  the  rhythm  by  divid- 
ing these  improvisations  into  lines.  Even  without  this,  however, 
the  resemblance  to  some  of  the  rudest  negro  improvisations  -is 
obvious,  —  not  the  best  ones,  preserved  as  standard  negro  songs, 
but  the  samples  given  by  observers  of  those  that  are  made  every 
day.  As  regards  the  melody,  too,  I  am  unable  to  resist  a  suspicion 
that  had  a  competent  musician  tried,  he  might  have  reduced  the 
apparently  tuneless  caroling  of  the  later  months  to  some  system 
with  very  primitive  intervals,  perhaps  like  those  detected  by  Miss 
Fletcher  in  Indian  chants,  or  even  like  those  of  bird-songs.  The 
pleasing  effect,  and  a  certain  resemblance  to  the  singing  of  birds, 
chiefly  to  the  musical  jargoning  of  blackbirds  in  the  fall,  is  what 
gives  me  this  suggestion ;  and  I  have  heard  an  ancient  Hawaiian 
incantation  that  had  a  good  deal  of  the  same  quality.  If  young 
children,  unable  to  master  our  melodies,  do  indeed  achieve  some 
more  primitive  sort  of  music,  while  improvising  words  in  a  manner 
that  unquestionably  resembles  that  of  the  rudest  races,  the  fact  is 
most  interesting.  And  such  a  primitive  musical  condition  should  be 
decidedly  more  evident  in  an  unmusical  child.  My  niece  may  be 
expected  to  inherit  very  little  musical  capacity:  her  father  and  most 
of  his  kindred  are  unusually  deficient  in  it,  while  her  mother  and 
her  mother's  kindred  have  it  in  but  moderate  degree. 

The  child  herself  was  perfectly  satisfied  with  this  singing.  One 
day  in  the  last  fortnight  of  the  year,  she  said  to  me,  "You  can't 
sing."  (This  I  had  often  told  her  when  she  wished  me  to  sing  for 
her.)  —  "No,  aunty  can't  sing."  —  "/can." 


The  Development  of  a   Child.  129 


5.   Interest  in  Hearing. 

The  first  interest  shown  in  any  sounds  was  undoubtedly  called 
out  by  chords  of  music.  The  next  may  possibly  have  been  in  the 
jingling  of  rattles,  which  from  the  third  month  (86th  day)  the  baby 
would  look  at,  and  finally  grasp,  with  an  appearance  of  interest,  but 
the  jingle  may  have  had  little  to  do  with  it:  they  Were  convenient 
objects  to  hold,  and  she  cared  more  to  mouth  than  to  shake  them; 
late  in  the  fourth  month  she  liked  to  bang  them  —  or  other  objects 
—  down  on  the  tray  of  her  high  chair,  —  an  amusement  that 
caused  great  glee  at  least  once  as  late  as  the  185th  day. 

At  three  months  also,  interest  or  curiosity  concerning  various 
sudden  sounds  began  to  be  shown  by  turning  the  head  to  look  for 
the  source,  as  noted  above  {Direction).  From  about  the  end  of  the 
third  month,  also,  it  was  a  great  joy  to  the  baby  to  be  propped  with 
cushions  in  a  high  chair  at  the  table  at  dinner,  where  she  would 
coo,  and  crow,  and  flourish  her  arms  wildly,  — ■  a  complex  pleasure 
into  which  the  lights,  faces,  voices,  and  rattling  of  dishes  doubtless 
entered. 

The  next  interest  in  sound,  beginning  the  109th  day,  was  in 
sounds  made  by  her  own  vocal  organs;  this  was  a  very  lively  inter- 
est, and  when  she  was  five  months  old,  her  chief  pleasure;  it  lapsed 
during  the  sixth  month,  but  reappeared  during  the  seventh,  and  with 
intermissions  continued  throughout  the  whole  period  of  this  record. 
But  the  pleasure  was  no  doubt  as  much  in  the  use  of  her  vocal 
organs  as  in  the  sound.  So  with  other  sounds  made  by  herself: 
the  action  was  a  large  part  of  the  interest.  Thus  the  ringing  of  a 
bell,  which  she  acquired  late  in  the  fifth  month.  At  the  end  of  the 
sixth  month  she  took  great  joy  in  this;  she  would  also  bang  the 
bell  down  hard,  till  her  chair  was  all  nicked,  winking  at  every  blow. 
Throughout  the  second  six  months,  from  time  to  time,  she  took 
great  pleasure  in  this  bell;  and  once  (265th  day)  when  it  had  been 
put  away  for  some  time  her  joy  in  recovering  it  was  intense. 
Again,  in  the  ninth  month  she  picked  up  the  practice  of  tooting 
into  a  horn,  cuff,  paper  tube,  in  imitation  of  our  blowing  on  a  horn; 
she  was  quite  interested  in  this  imitation  for  about  a  month,  but 
after  that  oftener  wished    to  see  us  do  it.     After  learning  in  the 


132  University  of  California.  [vol.  i. 

est  through  the  twenty-second  month ;  after  which,  I  think,  some 
change  in  the  pipes  stopped  the  squeaking. 

The  only  marked  interest  in  listening  to  a  sound  that  I  have 
noted  during  the  third  year,  occurred  in  the  one  hundred  and 
eleventh  week,  when  an  old  clock,  whose  apparatus  for  ringing  the 
hours  was  still  in  order,  was  turned  over  to  her  uses,  and  she  several 
times  sat  absorbed  for  nearly  half  an  hour,  listening  as  this  was  kept 
ringing.  This  pleasure  was  of  course,  however,  very  complex,  and 
consisted  more  in  seeing  the  apparatus  set  in  motion,  than  in  the 
simple  sound;  and  part  of  the  time  she  was  allowed  to  do  the  ring- 
ing herself. 

Although  my  notes  for  the  second  and  third  years  are  very  far 
from  being  exhaustive  in  such  matters  as  this,  it  is  certain  the  inter- 
est in  hearing,  apart  from  intellectual  inferences,  was  never  consid- 
erable. Sounds  were  chiefly  provocative  of  curiosity,  —  the  child 
wished  to  see  what  made  them,  how  it  was  done,  what  they  signi- 
fied, and  rarely  cared  to  listen  for  any  other  purpose. 

The  only  acoustic  experiments  recorded  are  in  the  eleventh 
month,  325  th  day,  when  she  rubbed  a  bottle  over  the  cane  seat  of  a 
chair  for  some  moments,  laughing  to  hear  the  sound;  and  in  the 
twelfth,  351st  day,  when  she  was  interested  in  rubbing  a  bottle 
across  the  matting  to  make  a  sound. 

A  few  incidents  showed  curiosity  about  sound  more  decidedly 
than  the  simple  turning  to  find  the  source.  In  the  twentieth  month, 
598th  day,  being  in  a  large,  bare  room,  a  gymnasium,  she  shouted, 
and  hearing  the  echo,  shouted  several  times  again,  listening;  then 
said,  "Noise!  Find  noise!"  She  was  serious,  troubled,  and  persist- 
ent about  it,  and  hunted  all  about  the  walls  and  entries,  saying, 
"Find  noise!  See  noise!"  At  last  she  fixed  on  a  row  of  Indian 
clubs  hanging  on  the  wall,  and  put  her  hand  on  them,  declaring 
that  was  the  noise;  but  she  was  not  satisfied  with  the  solution,  for 
she  asked  about  the  noise  and  wanted  to  "find"  it,  for  some  time 
after  we  left  the  building.  It  seems  probable  that  her  desire  to 
"see  the  noise"  did  not  indicate  an  idea  of  the  sound  as  a  visible 
thing  (though  this  was  my  impression  of  her  meaning  at  the  time), 
but  rather  a  confusion  of  the  word  "noise"  with  the  meaning 
"thine:  that  makes  the  noise."     But  that  her  consciousness  of  the 


shin.n-.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  133 

act  of  hearing  was  far  less  distinct  than  of  that  of  seeing  was  evi- 
dent when  some  two  weeks  later,  61 6th  day,  I  tried  the  experiment 
of  asking  her  what  she  did  with  her  eyes,  mouth,  hands,  nose,  ears. 
The  only  question  to  which  she  could  not  give  some  sort  of  an- 
swer was  as  to  her  ears,  —  she  had  not  connected  them  with  hearing 
in  the  smallest  degree.  We  tried  to  show  her  by  closing  and  open- 
ing her  ears  while  making  a  noise.  This  interested  her;  she  looked 
serious  and  absorbed,  remarked,  "Noise?  See?"  and  listened  when 
told  she  could  not  see  noise  —  it  went  right  in  at  her  ears;  but 
soon  said,  "  No  more,"  and  wished  the  experiment  to  stop. 

The  evidence  of  language,  also,  places  her  consciousness  of 
hearing  later  than  that  of  seeing.  The  general  word  "noise"  was 
not  used  till  the  twentieth  month,  "hear"  not  till  the  twenty-first; 
"music"  was  earlier,  in  the  seventeenth,  and  one  sound,  "squeak," 
was  known  by  name  in  the  sixteenth ;  while  at  a  year  old  she  first 
imitated  (with  a  little  teaching)  an  animal  sound,  and  in  the  next 
two  months  learned  to  name  several  animal  notes. 

I  am  permitted  to  add  here  some  interesting  notes  from  fellow-members  of 
a  "Child-Study  Section  "  of  the  Collegiate  Alumnae,  which  is  affiliated  as  a 
graduate  seminary  with  the  University  of  California. 

NOTES    BY  MRS.    LULU    MEDBURV  CHAPMAN,  A.  I!.,  UNIVERSITY  OF   CALIFORNIA 
I.  Direction  of  Sound. 

In  the  twentieth  month  the  boy  could  readily  determine  in  which  room  a 
bell  was  struck,  when  he  was  in  the  hall,  the  arrangement  being  thus: — 


Hall. 


2.    Music. 

In  the  twelfth  month,  while  unable  to  lift  his  feet  to  keep  time  without  losing 
his  balance,  the  boy  would  sway  his  body  up  and  down  in  time  to  well-marked 
music,  stopping  with  signs  of  impatience  when  he  lost  time.  A  little  later,  he 
would  clap  his  hands  in  time  to  music.  By  the  twentieth  month,  if  I  held  his 
hands  to  help  him  balance  himself,  he  could  keep  the  polka  step  for  six  measures 
at  a  time.  From  the  thirty-sixth  month  (he  is  now  in  the  fourth  year),  he  often 
beats  time  with  his  hand  while  his  sister  plays.  If  in  crossing  the  ferry  he 
chances  to  be  near  the  band,  he  beats  time,  apparently  unconsciously. 

In  the  thirty-eighth  month  I  made  the  following  experiments:  I  had  his  sis- 
ter play  a  march,  and  the  boy  clapped  his  hands  to  it,  keeping  good  time  when 
she  played  slowly;  but  when  she  played  the  same  thing  more  rapidly,  he  lost 


134  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

time  and  did  not  regain  it.  I  then  had  him  march  to  her  playing.  He  kept 
good  time  when  the  music  was  slow.  She  quickened  the  time,  and  he  lost  step, 
but  regained  it;  when  she  played  still  more  rapidly,  he  began  to  run  around  the 
table.  I  told  him  to  do  as  the  music  told  him,  and  to  keep  his  mind  from  wan- 
dering from  it  repeated  frequently,  "Listen  to  the  music."  After  three  trials 
he  learned  to  increase  the  rapidity  of  his  step  as  the  music  changed,  losing  step 
for  a  beat  or  two  at  each  change,  and  then  regaining  it.  He  succeeded  in 
keeping  step,  both  with  hands  and  feet,  to  a  well-marked  polka,  but  waltz  time 
utterly  confused  him. 

Although  the  child's  sense  of  rhythm  is  evidently  good,  and  he  likes  to  hear 
verse,  he  never  would  learn  it;  he  would  repeat  a  few  words,  then  go  rambling 
off  with  inventions  of  his  own.  So  far  as  I  recollect,  when  he  did  repeal  any- 
thing, he  readily  caught  the  meter.  At  thirty  months,  however,  when  I  was 
accustomed  to  sing  him  to  sleep  with  "Guide  me,  O  thou  great  Jehovah,"  he 
sang  himself,  "Guide  mamma,  O  thou  great  Jehovah," — an  obvious  sacrifice 
of  meter  to  his  understanding  of  the  sense.  Later,  he  would  sacrifice  sense, 
meter,  and  everything  else  to  getting  every  letter  and  sound  in  a  word  fully 
enunciated,  unless  he  could  lengthen  the  word  or  make  it  more  sonorous,  as 
i  liim-mun-ny  for  chimney. 

The  first  time  that  he  tried  to  sing  himselt  was  in  the  instance  mentioned  just 
above,  at  eighteen  months.  He  chanted  the  line  in  a  high  monotone,  prolong- 
ing the  lastsyllable,  without  time  and  without  tune.  For  about  four  months 
thereafter  he  made  occasional  similar  attempts,  then  hearing  the  older  chil- 
dren, who  had  been  to  hear  an  opera,  singing  a  great  deal,  he  made  more  fre- 
quent attempts  for  a  couple  of  weeks,  and  began  to  observe  the  time  some- 
what. After  this  he  sang  less,  and  at  three  years  did  not  keep  the  time  as  well 
as  in  the  thirty-fifth  month.  So  still,  in  the  middle  of  the  fourth  year,  he  loses 
the  time  by  his  punctilious  enunciation,  by  drawling,  and  by  a  tendency  to  turn 
everything  into  2/4  time.  He  cannot  follow  a  tune,  but  now  makes  some  effort 
to  do  so,  using  a  range  of  three  notes. 

3.  Verbal  Preferences. 

The  child  not  only  likes  blank  verse,  as  noted  above,  but  is  very  fond  of 
iingling  rhymes.  At  thirty  months,  following  the  older  children,  he  began 
rhyming  words,  as,  "I  see  a  wagon-pagon,"  or  oftener  would  play  on  the  chil- 
dren's names,  "Hester-pester,"  "Ada-tada,"  etc.  This  continued  several 
weeks,  then  was  given  up  entirely. 

In  the  twenty-fourth  month  I  first  noticed  a  fondness  for  alliteration,  which 
continues  in  the  middle  of  the  fourth  year.  For  some  three  weeks  in  the  thirty- 
second  month  he  distorted  his  speech  with  alliterations  to  a  painful  degree; 
as,  "He  halked  hown  the  heet,"  for  "He  walked  down  the  street."  (This 
has  reappeared  slightly  in  the  forty-third  month.) 

For  a  time,  some  certain  word  would  please  him  greatly,  and  would  be  used 
frequently,  regardless  of  sense.  In  the  twenty-fourth  month,  after  he  had 
heard  the  word  electric  used,  everything  became  "  laktic;"  all  cars  were  "  lak- 
tic  cars,"  we  had  "laktic  bread,"  etc.     In  the  thirty-second  month,  for  several 


siiinn]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  135 

weeks,  he  repeated  the  letteers  T-O-B  over  and  over  for  his  own  amuse- 
ment. Other  words  have  been  used  in  the  same  way.  As  they  conveyed  no 
possible  meaning  to  him,  he  must  have  b;en  moved  simply  by  a  fancy  for  the 
sound. 


NOTES  BY  MRS.  MAHEL  WALCOTT  ISEATTY,  li.  L.  UNIVERSITY  OF  CALIFORNIA. 
'  i .  Direction  of  Sound. 

Early  in  the  ninth  month  I  noted  that  my  boy  had  been  for  two  or  three 
months  noticeably  accurate  in  catching  the  direction  of  sounds.  Near  the  end 
of  the  eleventh  month  (332d  day)  he  was  alone  in  a  room,  and  I  in  another  on 
the  same  floor,  but  at  the  opposite  corner  of  the  house,  and  separated  from  the 
room  he  was  in  by  a  hall  and  another  room.  Hearing  him  fretting  I  called  to 
him  repeatedly  to  come  to  me.  He  stopped  fretting,  and  began  to  creep 
toward  the  sound  of  my  voice,  following  it  intelligently  into  the  intermediate 
room,  where  his  attention  was  attracted  by  something,  and  he  came  no  farther. 

2.  Music. 

The  child  has  been  at  all  times  fond  of  hearing  music.  In  the  first  six 
months  he  would  remain  very  quiet  listening  to  the  music  on  the  ferryboat. 
At  five  months  he  listened  a  long  time  attentively  to  the  piano,  and  has  never 
ceased  to  be  much  interested  in  hearing  it.  In  the  thirteenth  month  he  learned 
to  blow  a  mouth  organ,  and  was  always  delighted  with  the  sound;  before  he 
could  blow  it  himself  he  would  put  it  to  someone  else's  mouth  as  a  request  to 
have  it  blown. 

3.   Interest  in  Sounds. 

At  six  months  the  child  was  fond  of  squpezing  a  rubber  doll  given  him  a 
few  days  before,  to  make  it  cry.  He  seemed  always  to  like  the  sound.  In  the 
ninth  month  he  would  notice  the  train  bell  and  whistle  several  blocks  away. 
In  the  eleventh  month  anything  that  made  a  noise  was  preferred  as  a  plaything, 
and  drumming  on  a  tin  pan  was  a  favorite  amusement.  Loud  and  unfamiliar 
noises  at  this  time  (eleventh  month)  frightened  him.  So  odd  sounds  made  by 
people;  the  roar  of  the  incoming  train;  the  noise  of  the  scrubbing  brush.  When 
he  became  familiar  with  them,  he  would  lose  his  fear. 


THE  DERMAL  SENSES. 
Sense  of  Contact. 

i.  General  and  Local  Sensibility. 

From  the  first  day  the  baby  seemed  perfectly  aware  when  she 
was  touched  or  cuddled,  and  seemed  to  find  it  agreeable,  for  she 
would  stop  fretting  at  once. 

The  first  appearance  of  localized  sensibility  was  about  the  lips 
and  tongue.  I  saw  no  sign  definite  enough  to  be  recorded  of  her 
noticing  a  touch  (except  in  the  vague  way  mentioned  above)  till  the 
29th  day,  when  she  smiled  distinctly  at  a  touch  on  her  upper  lip, 
her  first  smile  for  a  specific  cause;  and  this  was  repeated  some 
minutes  later.  Again,  the  33d  day,  she  smiled  repeatedly  when 
her  mother  was  rubbing  a  speck  from  her  lip.  In  the  latter  part  of 
the  second  month  a  smile  could  almost  always  be  coaxed  by  rub- 
bing the  lip,  or  touching  the  cheek  with  a  finger  tip.  That  it  was  a 
smile  of  pleasure,  not  a  reflex,  seemed  evident,  as  the  eyes  smiled 
also,  and  once  she  smiled  two  or  three  times  after  such  a  touch, 
without  any  repetition  of  the  stimulus. 

In  the  seventh  week,  for  some  days,  she  experimented  with  the 
tongue  a  good  deal,  putting  out  and  withdrawing  the  tip  through 
pursed  lips.  In  the  eighth  month  she  returned  more  intelligently 
to  this,  and  from  time  to  time  during  the  month  had  a  habit  of  run- 
ning out  her  tongue  and  moving  it  about,  feeling  her  lips  and  trying 
its  motions. 

In  the  ninth  week,  when  she  was  held  close  to  anyone's  cheek, 
if  hungry,  she  would  lay  hold  on  it  and  suck,  but  if  not  hungry 
would  apply  her  lips  to  it  and  lick  it.  From  this  time  she  developed 
a  peculiar  delight  in  putting  her  lips  to  someone's  face  and  mouthing 
it,  which  lasted  throughout  the  year;  I  have  no  note  of  it  in 
the  second  year,  but  it  may  not  have  entirely  disappeared,  for  my 
notes  were  necessarily  less  exhaustive  as  the  phenomena  to  be 
observed  became  more  complex.  As  she  became  able  to  discrimi- 
nate between  people,  she  confined  this  mouthing  to  her  favorites, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Cliild.  137 

and  kissing  appeared  to  be  developed  from  it;  but  it  was  also  done 
a  great  deal  with  no  appearance  of  affection,  merely  rompingly;  she 
would  seize  on  a  face  that  she  could  reach,  as  she  was  held  in  our 
arms  or  laps  or  crept  over  a  lounge  or  bed  beside  us,  and  would 
mouth  it  with  demonstrations  of  gayety.  It  was  like  a  dog's  desire 
to  lick  one's  face,  in  caress  or  in  frolic. 

A  trick  of  trying  to  grasp  with  her  mouth,  even  to  reaching  out 
with  her  head  for  objects,  which  attended  the  earlier  stages  of  learn- 
ing to  grasp,  in  the  fourth  month,  seemed  also  connected  with 
superior  sensibility  in  the  lips. 

In  the  ninth  week  she  tried  a  good  deal  to  get  her  fists  into  her 
mouth;  before  this  she  had  sucked  and  mouthed  them  when  they 
reached  the  mouth  by  chance.  By  the  tenth  week,  she  had  them 
at  her  mouth  all  the  time;  by  the  twelfth,  sucked  her  thumb  and 
sometimes  her  fingers  constantly,  but  was  weaned  from  the  habit  by 
the  use  of  a  small,  closed  rubber  nipple,  which  was  then  gradually 
discarded.  After  she  could  grasp,  everything  went  to  her  mouth  for 
a  time,  during  the  sixth  month  and  on  into  the  seventh;  but  the 
habit  declined  perceptibly  in  the  seventh,  and  thereafter  gradually 
disappeared.  At  a  year  old,  she  rarely  put  things  to  her  mouth 
except  to  taste.  A  curious  exception  was  that  from  the  time  she 
first  had  a  chance  to  get  at  the  ground  (in  the  tenth  month,  285th 
day)  she  desired  to  put  sand,  gravel,  or  pebbles  in  her  mouth  in 
examining,  —  whether  to  taste  or  feel  I  could  not  tell.  This  lasted 
certainly  to  the  end  of  the  year,  if  not  longer;  and  again  in  the 
eighteenth  month  she  was  for  several  days  resolute  to  put  gravel  in 
her  mouth,  and  disobedient  about  it,  —  this  may  possibly  have 
been  connected  with  the  fact  that  her  teeth  were  visibly  troubling 
her.  Early  in  the  fourteenth  month  after  examining  small  new 
objects  for  many  minutes  with  eyes  and  fingers,  she  would  put 
them  to  her  mouth,  and  was  very  persistent  about  doing  it  and  dis- 
appointed when  prevented.  At  the  end  of  the  twenty-first  month 
there  was  for  a  few  days  a  curious  revival  of  the  habit  of  putting 
various  things  into  her  mouth,  and  when  pins  were  given  her  to 
stick  in  a  cushion,  or  pease  to  shell,  she  would  soon  stuff  them  into 
her  mouth,  —  an  evidence  how  unsafe  it  is  to  trust  to  apparently 
fixed  habits  of  babies  in  such  matters,  for  no  one  can  tell  what  un- 
expected whim  will  seize  on  a  baby  who  "never  puts  things  into 
his  mouth,"  or  "never  touches  the  stove." 


138  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  twentieth  month,  601st  day,  I  said  to 
the  child  several  times,  "Look  carefully,"  when  she  seemed  about 
to  make  a  mistake  in  naming  the  colors;  and  each  time  she  lifted 
the  cards  to  her  lips,  as  if  with  some  impulse  to  guide  herself  by 
the  feeling. 

The  first  sign  I  saw  of  any  special  sensibility  in  the  fingers  was 
the  64th  day,  when  I  noticed  a  pretty  way  of  holding  her  hands 
together,  as  if  when  they  touched  by  accident  the  baby  kept  them 
together  for  the  feeling  of  contact. 

The  twelfth  week,  she  fumbled  with  the  tips  of  her  fingers  on  a 
surface,  as  one's  hand  or  dress,  evidently  with  some  intention.  This 
developed  slowly  into  grasping  (under  which  head  I  hope  to  give 
more  detailed  notes  of  it),  with  a  constant  increase  of  evidence  of 
special  feeling  in  the  fingers.  After  she  learned  to  grasp,  she  habit- 
ually desired  to  touch  things  as  well  as  to  hold  them;  in  especial, 
she  often  put  up  her  fingers  to  touch  our  faces  or  hair. 

In  the  tenth  month,  279th  day,  I  saw  her  investigate  an  object 
with  her  forefinger  tip,  and  thereafter  noticed  that  either  the  fore- 
finger or  thumb  and  forefinger  were  preferred  in  feeling,  though  I 
scarcely  thought  the  preference  as  marked  as  in  grown  people. 
The  use  of  the  forefinger  in  pointing  was  developed  during  this 
month,  and  was  apparently  closely  related  to  its  special  use  in  feel- 
ing. 

The  earliest  incident  that  showed  sensibility  at  all  noticeable  in 
any  part  of  the  body  besides  lips  or  fingers  was  in  the  hand,  I  think 
the  back  of  it;  in  the  fourteenth  week,  I  kissed  her  hand  suddenly 
and  it  flew  up  as  if  by  reflex,  while  her  face  showed  surprise.  About 
the  same  time  she  was  amused  —  more  than  ever  before  —  by 
rapid  and  loud  kissing  of  her  hands,  across  fingers  and  palm,  as 
they  rested  half  shut. 

In  the  fifth  month,  133d  day,  she  was  deeply  interested  in  a  sen- 
sation of  touch  on  the  back  of  her  head  (see  below,  p.  143).  From 
the  twenty-third  week  to  the  end  of  the  year  from  time  to  time  she 
felt  curiously  with  her  fingers  about  her  head  and  neck,  —  the 
interest  doubtless  depending  on  the  double  sensation,  in  finger  tips 
and  in  the  part  touched.     These  incidents,  as  well  as  a  number  con- 


shinn.i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  139 

cerning  the  amusement  or  annoyance  excited  by  kisses  on  various 
parts  of  the  body  from  the  sixth  month  on,  are  given  below,  under 
other  heads.  So  too  see  below  concerning  experiments  in  the  sense 
of  touch  in  the  heels  (p.  144). 

From  the  sixth  month  on,  I  noticed  an  occasional  liking  to  press 
her  face  down  against  a  person,  perhaps  to  rub  her  nose.  In  the 
seventeenth  month  she  liked  to  rub  her  face  like  a  kitten,  over  our 
faces. 

2.  Discrimination  by  Touch. 

Discrimination  in  touch  sensations  was  for  months  shown  only 
in  special  likings  for  one  kind  or  another;  and  its  first  instance  was 
her  preference  for  hard,  smooth  objects  during  the  early  period  of 
grasping,  the  fourth  and  fifth  months.  For  feeling  either  with  mouth 
or  hands  wooden  or  metal  objects  were  preferred  to  soft  ones. 
The  use  of  the  soft  ball  at  this  period  for  educational  reasons,  as 
recommended  by  kindergartners,  seems  defeated  by  the  preference 
for  surfaces  that  offer  a  very  definite  resistance,  which  I  suspect  to 
be  the  rule  with  babies  in  the  first  six  months;  as  well  as  by  the 
greater  ease  with  which  the  cylindrical  form  —  a  rattle's  handle, 
c.  g.  —  can  be  grasped. 

In  the  first  year,  the  light  touch  of  kisses  on  various  parts  of  the 
face  and  body  (she  was  never  kissed  on  the  mouth  if  we  could  pre- 
vent it)  was  usually  regarded  by  the  baby  as  very  amusing.  The 
first  instance  of  this,  in  the  fourteenth  week,  has  been  mentioned 
above ;  and  late  in  the  sixth  month  she  was  repeatedly  delighted, 
laughing  aloud,  at  a  rapid  shower  of  kisses  on  her  legs;  in  the  same 
week  she  laughed  at  kisses  on  her  neck,  and  in  the  eighth  month  she 
was  very  fond  of  being  kissed  there,  and  would  hold  her  head  on 
one  side  a  long  time  to  have  it  done,  with  an  expression  of  enjoy- 
ment ;  so  at  intervals  to  the  end  of  the  year.  The  first  instance  I 
note  of  her  finding  kisses  annoying  is  in  the  nineteenth  month, 
when  she  twice  cried,  '"Way!"  (Go  away),  as  I  was  about  to  kiss 
her;  but  during  the  twenty-second  month  she  often  showed  annoy- 
ance at  them,  and  I  have  noted  sundry  such  protests  as,  "Don't 
kiss  Ruth,  —  hurts!"  "Don't  kiss  Ruth  little  baby  hand !"  once, 
"  Don't  eat  Ruth  neck ! "     If  it  struck  her  as  a  frolic,  she  was  amused 


140  University  of  California.  [Vol.  . 

as  before;  the  657th  day,  c.  g.,  she  was  amused  when  her  toes  were 
kissed  and  asked  me  to  "Kiss  'nother  little  baby  foot,"  and  the 
next  evening  when  undressed  offered  her  feet  again  and  dictated 
ways  in  which  she  wished  them  kissed,  —  first  one,  then  the  other, 
then  both  together,  on  toes  or  heels.  The  next  day,  after  declining 
to  have  her  face  kissed,  she  began  to  complain  that  "  Ruth  little 
knee  hurt,"  and  to  hold  it  up  to  me.  I  asked  if  it  wanted  kissing, 
and  she  assented,  and  offered  it  several  times  afterward  to  be  kissed, 
on  the  same  plea,  indicating  the  spot. 

In  several  other  ways  she  showed  about  this  time  a  marked 
annoyance  at  touches  on  the  skin.  In  the  twentieth  month  she 
objected  even  with  crying  —  which  was  most  unusual  —  to  having 
a  thermometer  bulb  applied  to  her  leg.  At  the  beginning  of  the 
twenty-second  month,  when  her  father  took  hold  of  her  bare  foot, 
she  cried,  "Papa,  don't  touch  Ruth  foot!"  several  times.  In  the 
last  week  of  the  month  she  lay  down  beside  me  to  be  entertained 
with  rhymes  and  stories,  and  wished  to  press  close  to  me,  but 
refused  to  let  me  put  my  arm  about  her  or  touch  her,  saying 
crossly,  "Don't!  "  —  "Don't  what?"  —  "Put  arm  on  Ruth  back."' 
During  the  whole  twenty-second  month,  in  fact,  restraint  or  med- 
dling, as  putting  hands  on  her  or  kissing,  was  the  usual  cause  of  a 
crossness  that  she  not  infrequently  showed.  "Don't!  Go 'way!" 
she  would  say  in  scolding  tones:  once  late  in  the  month  I  kissed 
her  hand  when  she  was  already  squirming  to  get  away  from  her 
father,  who  wished  to  hold  her,  and  she  snatched  her  hand  away 
fairly  snarling,  "Don't!" 

In  the  second  week'  in  the  month  her  mother  made  a  record 
that  the  child  was  walking  against  a  wind  that  blew  in  gusts,  and  at 
each  gust  she  would  scold,  "  Stop  !     Don't  blow !" 

In  this  irritability  toward  touches,  so  marked  during  one  month, 
there  was  evidence  of  a  heightened  nervous  susceptibility  that  was 
doubtless  general  and  not  confined  to  the  sense  of  touch;  it  evi- 
dently included  such  mental  elements  as  an  increased  feeling  of  per- 
sonality and  independence.  Thus  in  the  last  week  of  the  month  if 
her  foot  was  touched  or  taken  hold  of,  she  would  say  in  a  tone  of  dis- 
pleasure, "That  Ruth  foot!"  She  was  during  the  same  month  and 
most  of  the  year  not  at  all  chary  of  her  own  kisses  when  in  a 
mood  of  affection. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Cliild.  141 

In  the  sixth  month  she  was  very  fond  of  playing  with  strings, 
drawing  them  through  her  fingers;  and  during  the  seventh,  she  was 
more  likely  when  taken  in  arms  to  reach  for  one's  hair  than  to  try 
to  mouth  one's  face,  —  the  finger  sensation  being  now  preferred  to 
that  of  the  lips.  During  the  eighth  month  the  desire  to  finger  and 
pull  threads  of  hair  was  very  great,  and  it  lasted  with  some  intervals 
at  least  to  the  twenty-first  month ;  by  this  time  we  had  been  obliged 
to  break  her  of  the  habit.  During  all  this  time  she  would  scarcely 
go  to  sleep  in  her  mother's  arms  unless  her  fingers  were  moving  in 
her  mother's  hair.  Once  in  the  twentieth  month,  when  my  hair 
was  hanging  after  being  washed,  I  allowed  her  to  pull  it  a  little,  and 
she  was  very  happy  in  doing  so,  eagerly  threading  her  fingers 
through  it  and  parting  the  strands.  Even  now  in  the  middle  of  her 
fourth  year,  when  she  has  long  been  accustomed  to  regarding  it  as 
forbidden,  her  hand  will  occasionally  wander  to  our  hair  as  she  sits 
on  our  laps,  and  begin  threading  and  pulling;  the  feeling  seems  to 
be  exceedingly  pleasing  to  her. 

In  the  ninth  month  she  began  to  show  recognition  of  differences 
in  the  feeling  of  surfaces,  by  signs  of  curiosity  in  novel  ones.  On 
the  248th  day  she  felt  carefully  of  her  head  in  a  new  muslin  cap, 
as  if  recognizing  the  change  from  the  silk  ones  she  had  hitherto 
worn.  The  tenth  month,  285th  day,  she  was  greatly  interested  in 
the  novel  surface  of  the  gravel  walk,  which  she  had  discovered  by 
creeping  to  the  edge  of  the  lawn;  she  felt  it  over  with  finger-tips, 
and  rubbed  the  smooth  dirt  at  the  edge  to  and  fro  several  minutes 
before  she  began  to  carry  it  to  her  mouth.  To  the  end  of  the  year 
the  surface  of  the  ground,  which  she  had  been  allowed  little  chance 
to  feel,  was  an  object  of  much  curiosity,  and  every  time  she  was 
allowed  to  touch  it  she  investigated  in  the  same  way.  When 
allowed  the  same  day  to  creep  up  to  trees,  she  felt  of  the  bark  with 
marked  interest  in  the  new  surface.  The  eleventh  month,  315th 
day,  when  taken  in  her  uncle's  arms,  she  rubbed  her  fingers  over 
the  shaven  surface  of  cheek  and  chin,  novel  to  her,  with  curiosity. 
So  repeatedly  from  this  time  new  surfaces  of  cloth,  as  a  plush  chair, 
a  silk  dress,  were  examined.  Up  to  the  end  of  the  thirteenth  month 
at  least  she  would  always  notice  a  new  texture  in  our  dresses  as 
soon  as  she  touched  them,  and  pass  her  fingers  curiously  over  it. 


\_\2  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

In  the  fifty-fifth  week  the  zinc  around  the  kitchen  stove  attracted 
her  attention,  and  she  would  drop  down  to  rub  her  fingers  over  it. 
Once  in  the  nineteenth  month  she  was  eager  to  get  hold  of  a  china 
rose,  and  when  allowed  to  feel  it,  was  silent  with  surprise  at  the 
unexpected  hard  surface. 

Her  ability  in  the  twenty-fourth  and  twenty-fifth  months  to 
identify  by  feeling  the  solid  forms  she  was  familiar  with,  has  been 
mentioned  under  Form.  As  early  as  the  fourteenth  month  she 
could  name  my  features,  hands,  or  feet,  on  passing  her  hands  over 
them  in  the  dark,  and  move  her  hand  quite  straight  to  find  them; 
but  in  the  last  week  of  the  twenty-third  month  failed  in  an  inter- 
pretation that  required  memory  of  more  complex  sight-impressions: 
feeling  for  me  as  I  sat  on  a  lounge  in  the  dark,  she  found  rny 
feet  resting  on   the  floor,  ami  said  that  I  was  under  the  lounge. 

Just  at  the  close  of  the  twenty-third  month  I  was  one  night,  in 
her  parents'  absence,  sleeping  in  her  mother's  place  beside  her; 
before  light  she  climbed  into  the  bed  from  her  crib;  and  though  she 
touched  my  face  and  hair,  she  did  not  discover  that  it  was  not  her 
mother  till  daylight;  she  was  drowsy,  it  is  true,  yet  even  so  the 
sense  of  hearing  never  failed  to  detect  me  at  once  when  I  spoke 
under  such  circumstances. 

3.  Interest. 

The  earliest  interest  distinctly  shown  —  something  more  than 
simple  pleasure  in  a  touch  —  was  in  feeling  her  lips  with  her  tongue, 
seventh  week,  as  mentioned  above;  next,  in  getting  her  hands  into 
her  mouth,  ninth  week,  and  thereafter  in  putting  objects  into  her 
mouth,  and  in  mouthing  faces.  At  just  nine  weeks  appeared  some 
vague  interest  in  her  fingers  (see  above)  and  from  the  twelfth  week 
on  in  the  things  touched  by  them. 

The  interest  thus  far  was  of  a  vague  character;  in  the  fifth  month 
it  first  took  the  form  of  a  definite  and  strong  curiosity.  Up  to  this 
time  I  had  noticed  over  and  over  that  the  baby  did  not  in  any  case 
look  at  the  spot  of  any  peripheral  sensation,  or  put  her  hand  to  it 
(unless  rubbing  her  eyes  when  sleepy,  which  she  did  from  at  least 
the  beginning  of  the  third  month,  be  an  exception)  nor  did  she  in 
any  way  seek  the  source  of  any  touch  sensation.     She  never  looked 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  143 

curiously  at  her  hands  when  learning  to  grasp,  as  other  childen  do. 
On  the  133d  day,  however,  she  was  sitting  in  a  horse  collar  on  the 
floor,  and  bent  herself  over  the  back  of  it  till  the  back  of  her  head 
touched  the  floor.  She  righted  herself  and  tried  again,  with  her 
head  turned  sidewise  as  if  to  see  what  touched  her.  In  this  posi- 
tion she  failed  to  reach  the  floor;  she  righted  herself  and  tried  again 
a  full  dozen  times,  looking  at  me  in  the  intervals  with  an  expres- 
sion of  the  greatest  surprise  and  seriousness,  which  I  could  not  dis- 
sipate by  laughing  and  speaking  encouragingly.  Her  father  tried 
to  divert  her  from  the  experiment;  but  as  soon  as  he  would  let  her 
alone,  she  would  renew  it.  Finally,  as  she  began  to  seem  tired  and 
troubled,  her  mother  took  her  up.  She  kept  up  the  same  experi- 
ment for  some  days. 

About  a  month  after  this,  158th  and  159th  days,  she  chanced  to 
touch  the  side  and  back  of  her  head,  and  proceeded  to  feel  them 
over  with  a  grave  air  that  was  very  comical.  The  181st  day  her 
hand  came  in  contact  with  her  ear ;  she  became  at  once  very  seri- 
ous, and  felt  it  and  pulled  it  hard;  losing  it,  she  felt  about  her  cheek 
for  it,  but  when  her  mother  put  her  hand  back,  she  had  become 
interested  in  the  cheek,  and  wished  to  keep  on  feeling  that.  Dur- 
ing the  succeeding  days,  early  in  the  seventh  month,  this  feeling 
the  side  of  her  head,  her  ear,  and  cheek,  was  a  habit;  she  also  felt 
of  her  lips  and  gums  with  two  or  three  fingers  very  seriously.  The 
next  month,  234th  day,  she  ran  her  tongue  far  out  and  examined  it 
with  her  fingers,  putting  them  in  as  far  as  she  could,  and  this  was  a 
habit  for  some  days.  To  the  end  of  the  year,  she  would  from  time 
to  time  feel  over  her  head,  neck,  hair,  and  ear;  the  hair  she  dis- 
covered in  the  eighth  month,  222d  day,  while  feeling  for  her  ear, 
and  felt  it  over  and  pulled  it  with  great  curiosity.  In  the  eleventh 
month  she  had  a  habit  of  taking  her  head  in  both  hands  (placing 
them  in  the  neighborhood  of  the  parietal  sutures),  as  if  to  estimate 
its  shape  and  size,  —  a  trick  whose  meaning  was  never  clear  to  me. 

In  the  seventh  month,  206th  day,  she  rubbed  her  forefinger 
investigatingly  with  the  thumb  of  the  same  hand  (the  left)  looking 
at  them;  but  in  general  her  hands  did  not  attract  her  attention. 

If  such  investigations  of  her  own  features  continued  in  the  second 
and  third  years,  they  were  not  sufficiently  noticeable  to  have  kept 
my  attention. 


144  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

In  the  tenth  month,  when  just  forty  weeks  old,  she  chanced  to 
be  set  on  the  carpet  with  stockinged  feet,  and  began  rubbing  her 
heels  on  the  carpet,  laughing,  apparently  enjoying  the  friction. 
Thereafter  till  the  twelfth  month  she  would  from  time  to  time  thus 
rub  her  heels,  in  soft  kid  moccasins,  on  the  floor,  especially  on  sur- 
faces not  before  tested,  —  matting,  rug,  etc.,  —  as  if  feeling.  After 
thicker  shoes  were  put  on  her,  in  the  twelfth  month,  I  have  no  note 
of  it. 

In  the  fourteenth  and  fifteenth  months  she  was  greatly  interested 
in  finding  and  feeling  over  objects  in  a  dark  closet,  —  the  broom  or 
drawer  handles,  c.  g.,  —  and  then  having  the  drawer  opened  and 
seeing  them,  and  feeling  over  again  with  the  aid  of  sight.  The 
relation  between  the  impressions  given  by  the  two  senses  seemed 
to  appeal  to  her  curiosity. 

In  the  seventeenth  month,  498th  day,  having  pulled  at  a  twig 
that  flew  up  and  hit  her  nose  slightly,  she  repeated  the  experiment 
some  half-dozen  times.  In  the  nineteenth,  559th  day,  she  amused 
herself  by  bumping  her  chin  with  the  handle  of  a  bronze  dinner 
bell;  she  once  did  it  harder  than  she  meant  to,  and  broke  out  cry- 
ing and  complaining, but  in  a  minute  wanted  the  bell  back  and  con- 
tinued the  experiment  more  cautiously. 

Pain. 

1.  Sensibility. 

Until  the  sixth  month  no  hurt  happened  to  the  baby,  and  I 
made  no  experiments  in  the  direction  of  her  sensibility  to  pain. 
But  there  was  at  least  one  instance  in  which  I  should  have  ex- 
pected her  to  feel  pain,  and  she  evidently  felt  none.  This  was  early 
in  the  fourth  month,  97th  day,  when  she  began  to  pound  the  tray 
of  her  high  chair  with  her  right  hand.  She  would  pound  rapidly 
four  or  five  times,  apparently  hard  enough  to  bruise,  then  after  a 
few  seconds  repeat, — all  with  great  satisfaction.  To  save  her  hand 
someone  put  into  it  a  rattle,  consisting  of  a  sleigh  bell  set  on  a  strong 
strip  of  rubber,  and  she  continued  pounding  with  this  so  strongly 
that  the  sleigh  bell  split  and  flew  apart,  on  which,  the  broken  rattle 
being  removed,  she  went  on  gayly  pounding  with  her  own  hand, 
as  before.  I  certainly  could  not  have  struck  my  hand  so  hard 
without  pain.     Nor  could  the  violence  of  the  blow  have  been  ap- 


shjnn.]  The  Dcvcloptiniit  of  a  Child.  145 

parent  only,  for  the  mechanical  effect  on  the  sleigh  bell  gave  an 
unmistakable  test;  yet  no  bruise  was  visible  on  the  baby's  hand, 
which  seemed  incomprehensible,  for  whatever  her  insensibility  to 
pain,  I  should  have  supposed  the  blows  would  have  bruised  the 
fat  little  hand  by  their  mere  physical  effect. 

From  the  fourth  month  until  she  learned  to  creep  the  baby 
would  raise  her  heels  as  she  lay  on  her  back,  and  bring  them  down 
with  violence,  enjoying  it  greatly.  From  the  seventh  month  she 
destroyed  her  little  kid  moccasins  thus.  At  just  six  months  old, 
i82d  day,  she  banged  a  small  steel  bell  down  hard  on  her  leg  again 
and  again  without  any  sign  of  pain. 

In  the  sixth  month  she  received  her  first  hurt,  a  deep,  red 
scratch  on  her  finger.  She  cried  bitterly,  but  it  seemed  to  be 
mainly  with  fright,  and  she  was  soon  consoled,  and  never  showed 
any  discomfort  from  the  wound  after  its  first  infliction.  I  observed 
the  same  thing  thereafter  invariably  up  to  the  twentieth  month. 
Hurts  seemed  to  leave  no  soreness  in  the  skin,  and  were  forgotten 
after  the  first  cry,  even  while  their  physical  traces  remained.  On 
the  28th  day,  e.g.,  some  insect  stung  the  baby's  forehead,  possibly 
a  bee;  but  though  she  cried  out  sharply,  she  stopped  the  instant 
she  was  in  her  mother's  arms,  even  before  the  spot  had  begun  to 
redden,  and  though  it  swelled  and  showed  for  hours,  she  never 
thought  of  it  again.  Her  cry  seemed  to  have  been  more  of  fright 
than  pain. 

From  the  time  she  began  to  roll  about  freely,  in  the  eighth 
month,  she  bumped  her  head  against  the  furniture  almost  daily,  but 
often  would  not  cry  at  all.  She  would  look  sober  for  a  minute, 
then  if  laughed  at,  would  smile  and  go  on  with  her  play,  and  when 
she  did  cry  she  was  diverted  with  ease.  Sometimes  bumps  suffi- 
cient to  redden  the  skin  produced  no  cry,  especially  if  someone 
was  at  hand  to  speak  an  encouraging  word.  Once  in  the  eighth 
month  she  thumped  her  nose  and  lips  hard  with  a  little  bottle, 
without  seeming  to  mind.  For  a  few  especially  sharp  bumps,  and 
for  a  fall  on  her  nose,  and  another  that  cut  her  lip  so  that  it  bled 
freely  and  puffed  up  (all  these  from  the  eighth  month  to  the  end  of 
the  year),  she  cried  loud  and  hard,  and  was  not  readily  consoled; 
but  after  the  hurt  had  been  relieved  she  never  recurred  to  it,  and 
showed  no  sign  that  any  soreness  remained  in  the  place.     Yet  in 


146  University  of  California.  [vol.  i. 

one  of  these  falls  she  struck  her  forehead  so  sharply  that  the  mark 
is  distinct  now,  in  the  middle  of  the  fourth  year. 

In  a  single  case  she  showed  a  sensitiveness  that  seemed  equal 
to  an  older  person's.  This  was  when  her  nose  was  chafed  with  a 
cold,  and  she  cried  if  it  was  wiped.  Her  skin  is  unusually  thick 
and  strong,  and  was  never  chafed  except  on  this  occasion  in  the 
first  year,  and  perhaps  three  or  four  times  in  the  second  and  third. 

After  she  had  in  rolling  and  the  first  creeping  acquired  some 
knowledge  of  bumps,  she  seemed  more  ready  to  cry  over  them,  — 
whether  because  she  grew  more  sensitive  or  more  afraid. 

I  never  but  once  in  the  first  year  saw  her  show  any  sign  of 
sensitiveness  in  her  gums,  even  when  the  tooth  was  near  the  sur- 
face and  the  gum  purple  and  swollen,  and  I  was  not  sure  on  that 
occasion.  She  cried  out  three  times  when  biting  something  hard, 
but  each  time  there  was  something  else  that  might  have  caused  the 
cry.  In  the  77th  week,  however,  when  the  back  teeth  were  coming, 
her  gums  visibly  troubled  her,  and  her  fingers  were  much  in  her 
mouth. 

In  the  fourteenth  month,  399th  day,  she  slipped  her  hand  under 
a  descending  knife,  and  it  took  off  about  half  the  ball  of  her  little 
finger.  She  cried  for  half  an  hour,  but  once  consoled  was  very 
happy,  and  never  made  the  least  whimper  over  the  finger  again, 
though  it  was  some  two  weeks  in  healing.  So  in  sundry  other 
instances  in  the  second  year,  she  either  did  not  seem  to  mind  a 
pain  at  all,  or  forgot  it  immediately.  One  incident  in  the  seven- 
teenth month,  503d  day,  showed  that  a  sharp  pricking  was  felt, 
where  duller  pressure  was  not.  She  had  been  fretful  all  the  after- 
noon; finally  she  began  hitching  and  twisting  in  her  clothes,  and 
we  found  in  her  shirt,  between  her  shoulder  blades,  a  sharp  prickle 
of  barley  beard,  the  result  of  playing  in  barley  hay.  When 
relieved  of  this  she  became  much  more  cheerful,  and,  after  she  had 
had  her  dinner,  was  quite  herself,  and  played  about  for  an  hour 
happily;  yet  when  she  was  undressed,  it  was  found  that  all  this 
time  her  shoes  and  stockings  had  been  full  of  sharp  gravel,  and  the 
sides  and  soles  of  her  little  feet  were  deeply  dented  all  over,  without 
having  caused  her  any  apparent  concern.  A  single  pebble  denting 
a  sensitive  grown  person's  foot  a*  deeply  would  have  made  him 
uncomfortable,  and  the  child's  soles  were  not  yet  at  all  calloused. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  147 

In  the  twenty-fourth  month,  also,  prickles  from  the  barley  hay  in 
her  stockings  and  drawers  annoyed  her  almost  to  crying. 

In  the  twentieth  month  occurred  the  first  instance  in  which  a 
pain  seemed  to  linger.  She  had  burned  her  finger,  through  my 
carelessness  in  letting  her  put  her  hand  on  register  pipes  which 
were  much  hotter  on  one  side  than  I  had  on  testing  found  them  on 
the  other.  She  broke  out  crying  inconsolably,  and  the  usual  diver- 
sions, such  as  talcing  her  into  another  room,  showing  her  objects, 
etc.,  did  not  prevent  her  recurring  with  rising  sobs  to  her  finger. 
This  was  doubtless  in  part  because  of  the  shock  to  her  confidence 
in  me,  for,  when  I  said  in  her  hearing,  "  It  was  aunty's  fault,  too!" 
she  repeated  through  her  tears  over  and  over,  "Owgu  fau',  too!  " 
and  though  she  did  not  know  the  meaning  of  "fault,"  she  would 
not  have  taken  up  the  phrase  while  absorbed  with  her  hurt  if  she 
had  not  in  some  sort  guessed  its  bearing  on  the  grievance.  But 
her  continued  grief  must  have  been  also  in  part  because  the  pain 
lasted,  for  she  would  look  in  a  concerned  way  at  her  finger  hours 
after  if  she  chanced  to  hit  or  press  it. 

A  few  notes  of  the  second  year  record  her  crying  for  slight 
scratches  or  bumps,  such  as  ordinarily  she  did  not  mind  at  all. 
The  reasons  of  these  inconsistencies  were  not  easy  to  trace,  but 
they  are  common  enough  at  all  ages,  as  everyone  has  experienced 
in  his  own  case;  they  probably  depended  more  on  variation  of  general 
nervous  irritability  than  on  that  of  the  special  sense. 

In  the  third  year  I  believed  her  sensibility  to  pain  increased,  but 
scarcely  on  evidence  that  I  could  formulate.  My  notes  show  the 
same  rule  of  indifference  to  pain  or  quick  forgetting  of  it,  with 
occasional  instances  to  the  contrary.  In  the  latter  part  of  the 
twenty-fifth  month  she  received  a  burn  without —  so  far  as  we  could 
learn  —  making  any  cry  till  she  hurt  the  place  some  minutes  later. 
She  was  creeping  about  under  the  table  with  her  doll,  and  cried 
out.  Her  grandmother  supposed  she  had  hurt  her  hand  on  her 
doll,  and  the  child  stopped  crying  almost  at  once  and  went  on  with 
her  play.  Some  fifteen  minutes  later  I  came  downstairs,  and  talcing 
her  in  my  lap,  asked  what  it  was  I  had  heard  her  crying  about. 
She  began  to  whimper  a  little,  and  said  she  had  burned  her  hand. 
Where  ?  She  indicated  the  back-  of  her  wrist.  But  where  was  she? 
what  did  she  burn  it  on  ?     She  did  not  understand,  and  kept  saying, 


148  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

"There,"  pointing  to  her  wrist;  so  I  asked,  "Did  you  burn  it  on 
the  fireplace?  or  did  you  touch  a  hot  poker?  or  meddle  with  the 
lamp?"  She  answered  in  her  jargon  something  in  which  I  made 
out  that  it  was  the  lamp,  and  it  was  her  uncle's  lamp.  As  I  saw 
no  burn,  I  set  down  the  story  as  fiction,  suggested  by  my  question; 
but  some  minutes  later,  in  a  brighter  light,  I  saw  that  her  wrist  was 
really  blistered  just  where  she  had  indicated.  On  being  questioned, 
she  repeated  that  she  had  burned  it  on  her  uncle's  lamp.  "Where 
was  uncle's  lamp?"  —  "On  grandma's  sewing  machine"  (in  an 
adjoining  room).  He  had,  in  fact,  had  it  there  a  little  while  before 
she  began  to  cry,  and  he  remembered  that  she  had  run  in  and 
reached  up,  just  touching  its  standard,  and  he  had  called  to  her 
that  it  would  burn  her,  on  which  she  had  run  away.  About  five 
minutes  later  she  had  cried  out  when  under  the  table.  Setting  the 
lamp  back  on  the  machine,  we  called  her  in  to  show  us  how  she 
had  burned  her.  She  reached  her  hands  up  in  the  air,  and  said, 
"Just  so,"  reaching  about  to  the  standard,  a  foot  or  so  below  any 
place  where  she  could  have  got  a  burn.  Whether  she  had  really 
climbed  up  unperceived  and  burned  her  hand  on  the  lamp,  or 
whether  she  had  got  the  blister  in  some  unobserved  dive  at  the 
grate  or  poker,  and  confused  this  in  memory  with  the  warning 
about  the  lamp,  we  could  not  make  out;  but  it  was  evident  that  it 
had  not  brought  any  cry  at  the  time,  nor  until  she  rubbed  or 
bruised  the  spot  later.  The  knowledge  that  she  was  doing  some- 
thing forbidden  might  have  kept  her  silent  for  a  slight  hurt,  but 
not  for  a  really  painful  burn,  such  as  an  older  person  would  have 
found  this. 

In  the  twenty-eighth  month  she  showed  much  reluctance  to 
have  a  sore  finger-tip  dressed  or  meddled  with,  but  in  the  thirtieth 
she  let  me  take  a  sliver  out  of  her  finger  with  a  pin,  without  the 
least  resistance,  and  seemed  scarcely  to  perceive  the  prick.  In  the 
thirty-sixth  month  she  let  me  begin  to  take  one  out,  but  when  the 
needle  began  to  prick,  she  objected  and  drew  away  her  finger. 
After  a  distinct  explanation  that  it  would  hurt  somewhat,  but  that 
she  could  be  brave,  she  held  her  finger  perfectly  still  and  let  me 
take  it  out.  Later  in  the  same  month  no  flattery  about  her  bravery 
would  prevail  on  her  to  permit  a  similar  slight  pricking. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  thirty-fourth  month  she  had  a  bad  fall, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Cliild.  149 

of  perhaps  eight  or  ten  feet,  on  an  asphalt  floor,  and,  though  she 
escaped  grave  injuries,  her  face  was  severely  bruised.  She  cried 
most  of  the  time  for  about  an  hour  after  this  accident,  until,  what 
with  nausea  from  the  shock  and  some  slight  concussion  of  the 
brain,  she  became  drowsy;  but  waking  the  next  day  almost  free 
from  these  stupefying  effects,  still  seemed  surprisingly  indifferent 
to  the  terribly  cut,  skinned,  and  bruised  lips,  which  it  would  have 
cost  a  grown  person  real  effort  to  endure  with  the  cheerfulness  she 
showed.  She  evidently  did  not  awake  to  any  consciousness  of 
discomfort,  for,  as  I  was  told,  her  first  remark  had  nothing  to  do 
with  it,  but  was  about  her  kitten  mewing  outside,  and  the  next 
morning  she  climbed  merrily  into  her  mother's  bed,  then  putting 
up  a  finger  to  feel  her  lip,  broke  into  a  wail  of  disappointment,  "  It 's 
sore  yet,  mamma! " 

(In  the  fourth  year  she  has  grown  visibly  either  more  sensitive 
to  hurts  or  more  cautious  about  them,  but  on  the  whole  takes  them 
very  easily.) 

The  readiness  with  which  a  slight  sensation  of  pain  was  sub- 
ordinated by  interest  in  the  higher  sensations,  or  in  some  mental 
employment,  or  even  by  pride  in  her  courage,  is  evident  in  several 
of  the  above  instances.  It  was  noticeable  even  in  the  first  year, 
when  hurts  were  forgotten  the  instant  a  diversion,  as  going  out- 
doors, was  offered,  and  in  the  second  and  third  year  more  marked 
instances  occurred.  For  example,  early  in  the  seventeenth  month, 
493d  day,  the  child  thrust  her  arm  into  a  puppy's  mouth,  and 
accepted  his  playful  but  ungentle  biting  as  most  entertaining.  One 
day  in  the  eighteenth  month  she  was  much  interested  in  thumping 
together  two  large  glass  marbles,  but  soon  thumped  a  finger  be- 
tween them.  She  cried  loudly,  gasping;  but  when  her  mother 
caught  her  up,  she  gasped  out  between  her  wails,  "Ma'!  ma'!"  and 
it  proved  she  was  calling  for  one  of  the  marbles,  which  had  fallen, 
while  the  other  remained  tightly  clutched  in  her  little  hand.  When 
the  missing  one  was  picked  up  and  handed  her,  she  smiled  and 
began  beating  them  together  again,  and  played  thus  with  them  at 
intervals  for  a  long  time,  —  an  hour,  perhaps. 

In  the  twenty-fourth  month  she  persisted  in  running  up  to  the 
dog  and  jerking  his  ear,  till  he  snapped  at  her  and  grazed  her  hand 


1 50  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

When  I  reached  her  she  was  looking  at  her  hand  with  pleased 
interest,  and  remarked  to  me,  as  if  it  had  been  meant  for  an  enter- 
tainment, that  "  Ruth  did  pull  Muzhik  and  Muzhik  did  bite  Ruth  a 
little."  She  repeated  this  over  and  over  to  members  of  the  family, 
and  was  determined  to  run  back  and  try  the  experiment  again. 
Even  punishment  would  not  keep  her  from  it,  and  in  the  end  the 
dog  was  sent  away.  In  like  manner  in  the  thirty-second  month 
she  was  absorbed  in  interest  in  the  cats,  and  as  there  chanced  to 
be  no  gentle  cat  about,  she  wished  all  day  to  run  after  some  half- 
wild  ones,  indifferent  to  scratches.  On  one  occasion  she  thrust  her 
hand  into  a  bush  after  the  cat,  turned  around  to  call  to  me,  "  Kitty 
did  'catz  me  a  little  bit,"  then  dived  into  the  bush  again,  and,  as  I 
came  to  her,  looked  up  and  added,  "  Kitty  did  'catz  me  again,"  and 
showed  a  second  sharp  red  scratch  on  her  finger,  then  wished  to  be 
allowed  to  make  a  third  attempt  to  get  hold  of  the  cat. 

Her  indifference  to  pain  compared  to  her  desire  to  follow  her 
occupations,  had  some  amusing  illustrations  when  her  mother  began 
to  discipline  her  by  its  use  in  the  second  year.  The  first  instance 
of  this  that  I  find  recorded  was  in  the  sixteenth  month,  478th  day, 
when  the  child  persisted  in  snatching  at  the  poker,  till  her  mother 
tried  to  stop  it  by  snapping  her  fingers.  She  would  whimper,  but 
as  soon  as  released  would  run  to  the  poker  with  an  air  of  roguish 
persistence.  The  second  time  it  happened  she  ran  to  me,  looking 
up  pleadingly  as  if  for  backing,  and  holding  up  her  fingers  to  be 
kissed.  One  day  in  the  twenty-second  month  the  threat  of  a 
spanking  for  kicking  her  heels  against  the  lounge  did  not  stop 
her,  and  her  mother  slapped  her  leg.  She  set  up  a  small  factitious 
cry,  and  whimpered,  "Little  leg  hurt!  Poor  little  leg!  Have 
kiss!"  and  after  soliciting  kisses  for  it  from  her  mother  and 
grandfather,  went  back  and  kicked  again,  testing  her  mother. 
Slapped  more  sharply,  she  desisted,  crying  in  real  earnest,  but 
without  resentment.  Once,  however,  earlier  in  the  same  month, 
her  mother  slapped  her  cheek  for  mischievously  biting,  and  .the 
child  cried  in  earnest,  kept  indicating  her  cheek  with  her  finger  as 
if  it  smarted,  and  did  not  stop  till  the  interesting  sound  of  the  water 
running  out  of  the  bathtub  diverted  her  attention. 

During  the  first  year  the  skin  seemed  entirely  insensitive  to 
mosquito  bites;  by  the  fifteenth  month  she  found  them  annoying, 
and  in  the  third  year  they  caused  great  irritation. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  \  5  1 


2.  Interest. 

While  pain  —  up  to  a  certain  point  —  was  very  easily  subordi- 
nated to  more  mental  interests  in  the  child's  attention,  it  yet  seemed 
to  me  to  rouse  in  her  a  kind  of  interest  as  an  experience  that  no 
other  sensation  did;  it  was  more  self-conscious  than  any  other, — 
as  perhaps  it  is  in  everyone,  and  necessarily.  The  earliest  evidence 
I  saw  of  this  was  in  a  certain  power  of  the  imagination  over  it;  her 
idea  of  the  hurt  could  be  easily  exaggerated  by  sympathy,  or  her 
memory  of  it  confused  by  suggestion  coming  from  herself  or  others. ' 

Thus  in  the  latter  part  of  the  thirteenth  month,  390th  day,  she 
fell  and  bumped  her  chin,  and  was  brought  upstairs  to  me  to  be 
consoled.  I  took  her  in  my  lap  and  said  sympathetically:  "  Did 
Ruth  fall  down  and  hurt  her  ?  Poor  little  girl !  "  Her  face  assumed 
a  very  rueful  look.  "Where  did  it  hurt  her?  "  She  put  her  hand 
on  her  forehead,  the  place  where  she  usually  got  her  bumps.  "  Shall 
aunty  kiss  it  ?  "  She  nodded  and  was  comforted.  I  tried  this  three 
times  again  during  the  afternoon,  and  each  time  she  looked  rueful, 
and  indicated  the  same  spot  on  her  forehead  as  having  been  bumped. 

The  efficacy  of  the  remedy  of  kissing  in  all  cases  of  slight  injur)' 
—  even  when  the  kiss  was  philosophically  administered  by  herself  — 
is  another  illustration  of  the  close  connection  of  imagination  with 
this  particular  sensation. 

In  the  middle  of  the  fourteenth  month,  408th  day,  she  burned 
her  tongue,  seizing  too  hastily  upon  her  potato.  Next  day  she 
showed  little  memory  of  it,  could  not  tell  where  she  burned  her, 
and  was  not  interested  in  the  subject;  but  three  days  later,  when 
the  conditions  were  repeated,  the  potato  before  her  again,  and  her 
mother  warned  her,  "Wait,  —  it  is  hot,"  I  asked,  "Does  n't  Ruth 
remember  how  the  potato  burned  her?"  and  she  nodded  and  put 
her  finger  to  her  mouth  to  indicate  the  spot.  As  late  as  the  446th 
day,  hearing  someone  speak  of  burns,  she  put  her  finger  to  her 
mouth  with  a  rueful  sound,  to  indicate  that  that  was  where  she  was 


1  It  is  to  be  considered  here,  however,  that  suggestion  was  brought  to  bear 
on  this  sensation  as  it  was  on  no  other,  through  our  sympathizing  or  our 
making  light  of  hurts. 


152  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

burned.  But  as  a  rule  she  could  at  this  time  remember  being  hurt, 
but  not  the  place  where  she  hurt  herself  In  the  first  week  of  the 
fifteenth  month,  429th  day,  having  bitten  her  tongue  or  finger,  she 
put  on  a  crying  face  and  lingered  on  the  verge  of  crying,  looking 
at  her  mother  and  me  and  visibly  making  demand  on  us,  till  I  went 
around  the  table  to  "kiss  the  place."  She  could  not  show  me 
where  it  was,  and  did  not  seem  to  understand  till  I  went  back  to 
my  seat,  then  she  held  up  the  thumb  and  wished  me  to  kiss  that 
and  all  the  other  fingers,  and  was  not  satisfied  till  I  did  this  for 
both  hands.1  Possibly  the  sense  of  location  in  her  feelings  was 
weak,  and  easily  lost.  This  may  have  had  to  do  with  an  incident 
of  the  twenty-sixth  month,  when  she  was  sleepy  and  fretful  at 
dinner,  crying  and  saying  that  her  leg  hurt  her  in  a  certain  place 
which  she  indicated,  though  no  sign  of  trouble  could  be  found 
there.  She  got  over  the  grievance  without  visible  cause,  and  was 
very  sunny. 

The  next  indication  of  the  way  in  which  her  mind  occupied 
itself  with  the  sensation  of  pain  was  a  curiosity  concerning  it,  and 
a  disposition  to  experiment  with  it  and  dramatize  it,  —  experiment 
and  pretense  being  so  mixed  that  I  cannot  separate  them. 

This  I  first  noticed  in  the  fourteenth  month.  We  had  been  in 
the  habit  of  warning  her  against  sharp  articles,  as  pens  and  scissors, 
by  showing  her  with  gentle  pricks  and  demonstrations  of  concern 
how  they  would  "hurt  the  baby."  She  would  now  beg  us  to  do 
this  when  she  caught  sight  of  the  articles.  In  the  fifteenth  month 
she  was  very  fond  of  pricking  herself —  sometimes  sharply  —  with 
a  hairpin,  or  pencil,  or  a  pin  begged  for  the  purpose,  and  she  wished 
us  to  pretend  to  be  much  concerned.  This  was  in  part  mere  imi- 
'  tion  of  our  small  drama  with  the  scissors  or  pens;  but  within  the 
month,  about  a  week  later,  she  began  to  dramatize  bumping  he 
head  in  like  manner,  knocking  it  carefully  against  something  ana 
coming  to  have  it  kissed.  She  was  very  persistent  at  this,  and 
would  keep  it  up  till  her  forehead  was  quite  red,  laughing  over  it 


1  It  is  likely  that  the  behavior  in  this  particular  instance  was  due  to  curiosity, 
to  mere  freak,  possibly  to  a  vague  following  out  of  suggestion,  rather  than  to 
real  confusion  as  to  the  place  of  the  hurt. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  153 

with  the  utmost  delight.  These  two  pretenses,  or  experiments, 
were  favorite  amusements  for  six  weeks  at  least,  with  recurrences 
later;  and  when  her  mother  pretended  to  sympathize  and  kiss  the 
place,  the  child  would  laugh  aloud  till  she  was  tired  out.  In  the 
sixteenth  month  she  pretended  to  pinch  her  finger  in  a  folding  rule, 
deliberately  thrusting  it  in,  then  holding  it  out  to  be  kissed;  and 
at  another  time,  touching  by  chance  a  potato  that  was  not  hot,  she 
began  to  complain  and  hold  up  her  finger  to  be  kissed,  and  so  sev- 
eral times  would  touch  the  potato,  draw  back  with  a  whimper,  and 
hold  out  the  finger. 

She  wished  also  to  try  these  experiments  on  others,  and  doubt- 
less got  some  valuable  material  of  consciousness  from  the  difference 
between  the  sensations  when  the  subject  was  herself  and  when  it 
was  another.  My  first  note  of  this  is  in  the  fourteenth  month. 
She  was  trying  experiments  in  pain  on  herself  with  a  sharp  hairpin, 
and  unexpectedly  turned  and  tested  it  on  me,  and  my  movement 
and  exclamation  delighted  her  greatly.  Late  in  the  eighteenth 
month,  the  539th  day,  she  seized  my  hand  and  pinched  the  skin 
on  the  back  sharply.  When  I  remonstrated,  she  admonished 
me  by  a  little  squeal  of  what  she  wished  me  to  do.  Finding  me 
unsatisfactory,  she  pinched  her  own  hand  and  gave  the  little 
squeal  herself  at  each  pinch;  but  soon  desisted,  apparently  not 
liking  the  feeling.  In  the  nineteenth  month,  556th  day,  she  wished 
to  beat  and  scratch  my  face,  and  when  I  would  not  allow  it  she 
experimented  on  her  own  face  quite  severely,  without  seeming  to 
mind. 

For  a  couple  of  weeks  at  the  end  of  the  fourteenth  and  begin- 
ning of  the  fifteenth  month,  she  would  bump  her  doll's  head  and 
bring  it  to  be  kissed. 

Once  in  the  twenty-first  month,  630th  day,  she  examined  pity- 
ingly a  completely  healed    scratch,  and   observed   that    her    litt1 
thumb  was  sore. 

As  the  child  was  not  in  general  imaginative  or  imitative,  these 
things  indicated  the  more  decisively  that  the  exciting  and  dramatic 
nature  of  the  whole  episode  of  being  hurt  —  the  pain,  the  outcry, 
the  hastening  to  console  —  had  made  a  marked  impression  on  her. 
With  this,  too,  was  doubtless  in  part  connected  the  crying  self- 
consciously as  a  bid  for  sympathy,  which  was  somewhat  common 


154  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

from  the  ninth  month,  and  appears  in  several  incidents  under  various 
heads.1 

TEMPERATURE   SENSE. 

In  the  first  year  the  baby  was  never  allowed  to  experience  any 
avoidable  variation  from  an  equable  warmth,  and  I  have  record 
only  that  she  never  gave  any  sign  of  discomfort  if  her  hands  or  feet 
grew  cold,  nor  in  unusual  heat.  She  was  always  overjoyed  on  hot 
days  (in  the  second  six  months)  to  have  part  of  her  clothes  off,  but 
the  feeling  of  muscular  freedom  had  much  to  do  with  this.  I 
noticed  on  one  excessively  warm  day  in  the  eleventh  month  that 
she  showed  no  fretfulness  or  languor  from  the  heat,  —  which  was 
hard  for  grown  persons  to  support,  —  but  continued  to  creep,  stand, 
and  scramble  about  with  her  usual  inexhaustible  activity,  while  the 
perspiration  stood  in  drops  on  her  face,  and  her  hair  was  wet.  She 
rejoiced  intensely,  however,  in  having  her  face  and  hands  bathed 
in  cool  water.  Several  times  in  the  second  year  I  thought  her  irri- 
table or  depressed  on  a  hot  day;  yet  on  other  such  days  I  noticed 
nothing  of  the  sort. 

The  first  indication  of  dislike  of  the  general  sensation  of  cold- 
ness and  desire  to  be  protected,  may  have  been  in  the  last  week  of 
the  nineteenth  month,  when  she  insisted  on  being  tucked  up  closely 
in  bed,  saying,  "Co'.  Worn."  (I  am  cold.  I  must  be  made  warm.) 
I  thought,  however,  that  this  was  rather  imitation  than  a  real  state- 
ment of  her  sensation,  and  she  did  not  seem  cold.  In  the  first  week 
of  the  twenty-third  month,  while  driving  one  day,  her  mother  sug- 
gested that  she  have  her  mittens  on.  "Yes,  Ruth  hand  be  [will  be] 
cold,"  the  child  answered.  The  same  night  her  father  came  home 
and  found  her  in  bed,  and  after  sitting  up  to  let  him  kiss  her,  she 
wished  to  be  covered  up  again  and  let  alone,  and  said,  "  Ruth  get 
cold."     She  began  in  the  twenty-fourth  month  to  cover  up  her  doll 

1  Notes  on  organic  pain  may  best  be  placed  here,  for  the  sake  of  complete- 
ness. The  child's  health  has  been  so  perfect,  however,  that  there  is  prac- 
tically nothing  to  record  under  this  head.  During  the  first  two  months  she 
was  a  few  times  fretful  with  colic,  and  once  cried  hard,  but  was  always  quite 
easily  relieved  ;  and  once  near  the  end  of  the  tenth  month  she  had  colic,  and 
made  a  great  fuss,  crying  for  an  hour  or  two  with  short,  sharp  cries,  as  if  in 
great  pain.  The  indications  were,  however,  that  it  was  not  a  severe  colic, 
and  that  she  was  a  good  deal  affected  by  fright  at  the  unaccustomed  sensation. 
I  find  no  other  note  concerning  organic  pain. 


siiinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Cliild.  155 

lest  it  should  get  cold,  and  this  was  for  at  least  three  months  a 
persistent  play,  holding  its  own  over  other  forms  of  pretending  to 
a  marked  degree.  When  she  piled  up  books  once  in  the  twenty- 
seventh  month,  she  commented,  "  Milicent,  I  going  to  build  nice 
house,  so  I  keep  nice  and  warm.  Milicent!  Milicent!  Milicent! 
I  going  to  build  nice,  warm  house."  Whether  in  all  this  there  was 
anything  more  than  imitation,  —  any  indication  that  the  sensation  of 
general  coldness  had  made  an  impression  on  her,  and  interested  her 
in  the  idea  of  shelter  from  it,  I  very  much  doubt.  In  a  hundred 
cases  she  wished  to  run  about  without  wraps  in  cold  air,  even  when 
her  hands  had  grown  cold  to  the  touch 

The  first  instance  of  sensitiveness  to  a  local  cold  touch  that  I 
noticed  was  in  the  seventeenth  month ;  but  the  sensitiveness  would 
doubtless  have  appeared  much  earlier  had  any  experiment  been 
tried.  On  this  occasion  the  child  mischievously  spilled  some  water 
from  a  cup,  according  to  a  troublesome  trick  she  had  at  the  time. 
As  it  chanced,  she  had  called  for  a  drink  at  a  moment  when  she 
was  naked,  and  the  cold  drops  trickled  unexpectedly  down  the 
front  of  her  body.  She  sprang  up  with  comical  surprise  and  dis- 
may, dropping  the  cup,  and  ran  across  the  room,  crying,  "  Spill ! 
spill!"  and  pointing  out  with  both  forefingers  the  course  of  the 
injury;  and  she  was  for  some  time  broken  of  the  trick  of  spilling 
water.  Yet  in  the  twenty-first  month,  when  bathed  in  the  large 
bath,  she  liked  to  have  the  cold  water  faucet  turned  on,  and  to  stand 
near  it  and  thrust  her  body  or  knee  forward,  so  as  to  be  just  grazed 
by  the  stream.  She  would  shrink  back  laughing,  then  invite  the 
little  cold  shock  again. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  nineteenth  month,  the  574th  day,  her 
mother  told  me  she  put  the  child  in  a  bath  at  980  Fahr.,  her  usual 
bath  being  88°.  The  child  at  once  said,  "  Hot!"  and  "  Burn  baby!" 
and  was  unwilling  to  sit  down  and  splash;  but  otherwise  did  not 
object  to  it,  and  cried  when  taken  out.  The  next  day  the  warm 
water  failed  by  some  accident,  and  her  bath  was  io°  too  cold, — 780 
instead  of  88°.  The  instant  she  was  put  in  she  looked  a  little  dis- 
mayed, then  said,  "  Cold!"  Urged  to  sit  down,  she  hesitated,  then 
slipped  and  fell,  her  face  going  partly  under  water.  What  with 
cold,  astonishment,  and  slight  strangling,  she  did  not  like  it  much; 


156  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

and  though  she  walked  about  in  the  water  without  exactly  objecting, 
she  responded  readily  to  my  suggestion  that  I  should  take  her  out. 
When  she  found  the  bath  was  over  prematurely,  she  began  to 
whimper  and  beg,  "Bath  more!  "  Reminded  that  it  was  too  cold, 
and  asked  if  she  wanted  to  go  back  into  it,  she  said,  "No,"  but 
began  to  beg  again  for  her  bath,  and  seemed  keenly  disappointed 
at  the  spoiling  of  it. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  twentieth  month,  climbing  up  on  a  bench 
warmed  by  the  sun,  she  commented,  "  Worn!  "  as  soon  as  her  knee 
was  fairly  on  it.  On  the  627th  day,  late  in  the  twenty-first  month, 
as  I  sat  with  her  on  a  flat  cellar  door  that  was  partly  in  the  sun, 
she  asked,  "Aunty  sit  here?  "  pointing  out  a  sunny  place.  —  "  No,  I 
will  sit  down  over  here,  thank  you,"  I  said.  —  "That  cool?"  she 
asked.  —  "Yes,  it  is  cool  here,  and  very  warm  over  there."  —  "  That 
warm?"  she  asked  again,  then  came  over  and  felt  the  boards  by 
me.  —  "  Don't  you  feel  that  it  is  cool  ?  "  I  asked.  —  "  No."  "  Go  feel 
over  there  where  the  sun  is  shining,  and  see  how  warm  it  is."  She 
obeyed,  and  when  I  asked,  "  Is  n't  it  warm?  "  answered,  "Yes." 

Late  in  the  twenty-third  month  she  experimented  on  the  warm 
lamp  shade,  touching  it  with  her  finger-tip  and  saying,  "That  hot;" 
then  she  picked  up  one  of  her  large  wooden  beads  and  applied  it 
to  the  shade,  then  felt  it,  and  said,  "That  don't  get  hot." 

In  the  third  year  she  rather  disliked  milk,  and  drank  it  only 
under  some  urgency.  Once  in  the  latter  part  of  the  thirtieth  month, 
shortly  after  she  had  thus  reluctantly  swallowed  a  little,  I  asked 
her,  "Why  are  you  wriggling  so,  Ruth?"  —  "That  milk  makes  my 
stomach  cold." 

In  the  first  week  of  the  thirty-third  month  she  once  had  a  hot 
water  bag  at  her  feet  on  account  of  an  indigestion.  The  next  night 
she  wanted  the  bag  again,  but  rather  as  a  whim  than  because  of 
real  liking  for  the  sensation,  for  she  soon  begged  to  have  it  at  her 
head,  saying  that  her  head  was  cold,  and  that  sometimes  when  her 
mamma  had  a  headache  she  had  the  hot  water  bag  at  her  head. 
However,  she  often  asked  for  the  bag  after  that,  and  seemed  really 
to  enjoy  the  feeling. 

In  the  thirty-fourth  month,  when  camping  beside  a  Sierra  river, 
her  mother  noted  her  comment  after  experience  of  the  water,  "That 
water  is  very  awfully  cold !  " 


sinNN.j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  157 

OTHER    DERMAL    SENSATIONS. 

From  the  earliest  months  the  baby  seemed  sensitive  to  the 
feeling  of  wetness,  and  unless  strongly  diverted  invariably  com- 
plained at  once  if  her  clothes  were  wet;  her  bath,  however,  was 
from  the  very  first  one  of  her  most  intense  enjoyments.  In  the 
nineteenth  month  she  objected  to  kisses  that  they  were  "wet." 

In  this  same  month,  seeing  a  high  light  on  a  black  silk  dress, 
she  cried,  "Wet!"  and  was  surprised  on  feeling  it,  and  commented, 
"Wet?  no?"  Thereafter  she  would  try  to  find  by  feeling  whether 
the  grass  was  wet,  and  would  rub  her  hands  on  it,  but  then  look  at 
them  to  see  if  they  were  wet,  not  trusting  to  feeling.  In  the  twenty- 
first  month  she  would  ask,  "That  grass  dry?"  before  stepping  on 
it,  and  bend  down  to  rub  it  with  her  little  hand,  as  she  had  seen 
us  do;  and  if  it  was  decidedly  wet,  she  could  tell,  and  would  say, 
"That  grass  wet."  The  last  day  of  the  twentieth  month  she 
observed,  ''Hands  wet!"  after  dipping  them  in  the  bath  tub. 

Tickling  her  was  not  usually  allowed;  nevertheless,  in  the  early 
part  of  the  seventeenth  month,  her  grandfather  tickled  her  a  few 
times,  and  she  took  ecstatic  delight  in  the  sensation.  She  would 
throw  herself  back  in  her  grandmother's  lap,  offering  her  chest  and 
neck,  and  beg  him  to  do  it.  When  he  did  it,  she  would  shout  with 
laughter  that  seemed  not  reflex  but  genuine;  and  when  after  half 
a  dozen  times  he  desisted,  she  begged  her  grandmother  and  me  to 
continue  it. 

WORDS    OF    DERMAL    SENSATION. 

It  has  seemed  to  me  best,  for  the  sake  of  easier  comparison,  to 
bring  together  here  all  notes  concerning  the  expression  in  language 
of  the  sensations  of  this  group,  rather  than  to  distribute  them  under 
the  several  heads. 

In  the  thirteenth  month  the  child  seemed  to  understand  the 
words,  "  Hurt  the  baby,"  or  "Burn  the  baby,"  for  she  would  avoid 
the  stove  when  thus  warned,  and  was  satisfied  when  denied  sharp 
things  with  the  explanation  that  they  would  hurt.  In  the  same 
month  she  seemed  to  understand  the  question,  "Where  did  it 
hurt?"  for  she  answered  by  indicating  a  place. 

The  words  of  temperature  sensation,  cold  and  warm  ("coo" 
and  "wow,"  later  "co"'  and    'worn";  were  the  first  used,  in  the 


158  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

fifteenth  month,  but  I  doubted  if  they  were  used  with  any  com- 
prehension :  -warm,  e.  g.,  may  have  meant  being  covered  up  in  bed, 
cold  being  uncovered.  The  direction,  "Warm  your  hands,"  was 
obeyed  by  holding  them  up  to  the  fire.  In  the  eighteenth  month 
the  child  said,  "Warm,"  when  her  wraps  were  put  on,  when  covers 
were  spread  over  her,  and  when  she  held  her  hands  to  the  fire  or 
saw  something  placed  near  it;  and,  "Hot,"  when  her  food  was  too 
hot,  or  she  was  thinking  of  touching  the  stove.  She  would  look 
at  her  mush  and  ask,  "Too  ho'?  — Tooco'? —  De  ri'  [just  right]?" 
This  formula  was  picked  up  from  her  mother's  comments  when 
testing  the  water  of  the  bath.  She  understood  perfectly  when  told 
her  food  was  "too  hot,"  and  did  not  wish  to  touch  it.  The  first 
time,  however,  that  I  was  certain  she  associated  the  words  cor- 
rectly with  the  sensations  was  when  she  used  them  of  her  bath  in 
the  last  week  of  the  nineteenth  month  (p.  155).  There  was  no  doubt 
that  after  this  she  used  the  words  intelligently  when  commenting 
on  a  warm  or  cold  contact;  but  whether  the  somewhat  different  use 
in  "Little  bit  cold  outdoors"  (twenty-first  month)  and  "Wind  cold" 
(twenty-fourth  month)  was  much  more  than  echo,  I  cannot  tell. 
See  also  the  use  of  hot  to  describe  a  disagreeable  taste  in  the 
nineteenth  month  (p.  162). 

Meanwhile  the  words  bump,  burn,  and  scratch  had  appeared, 
the  one  in  the  fifteenth  month,  the  others  in  the  sixteenth.  How 
far  they  described  sensations  it  is  hard  to  say:  the  child  would  put 
her  hand  to  her  head  in  saying,  "Bump,"  and  show  a  burn  or 
scratch  in  naming  these  injuries.  When  hot  was  acquired,  in  the 
eighteenth  month,  it  was  closely  associated  with  burn,  and  usually 
used  with  it:  thus,  "Hot.  —  Burn,"  would  be  said  of  a  stove.  The 
word  hot  may  easily  have  been  to  the  child's  mind  descriptive  of  a 
pain  sensation  rather  than  of  a  temperature  one.  The  first  general 
expression  of  pain  in  words  was  an  indirect  one:  in  the  twenty- 
second  month  the  child  would  say,  if  scratched  or  bumped,  "Poor 
Ruth!  O  poor  Ruth!"  But  a  week  later,  in  the  same  month,  the 
general  word  hurt  was  acquired,  and  the  indirect  expression  dropped. 
Hurt  was  used  both  actively  and  passively:  "Hurt  Ruth  head," 
"Ruth  lip  hurt."  A  slight  tendency  at  first  to  use  it  loosely  of 
any  disagreeable  feeling  was  shown  by  the  protest,  "Don't  kiss 
Ruth,  —  hurts;"  but  this  was  the  first  day  the  word  was  used, 
and  did  not  continue.      Sore  appeared  in  the  twenty-first  month. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  159 

Wet  appeared  in  the  nineteenth  month,  and  dry  in  the  twentieth. 

The  first  word  of  touch  sensation,  smooth,  also  appeared  in  the 
twentieth,  but  the  only  instance  I  have  of  its  use  referred  to  smooth- 
ness perceptible  by  the  eye  rather  than  by  touch.  The  general 
word  feel  was  used  in  the  last  week  of  the  month,  but  only  when 
referring  to  organic  sensation,  as  "feel  better,"  "feel  sick,"  and  to 
the  act  of  feeling  with  the  finger.  I  did  not  notice  it  in  any  phrase 
clearly  referring  to  dermal  sensations  (as  "feel  cold,"  "feel  sore," 
"feel  smooth")  within  the  second  year. 

NOTES    BY    MRS.    BEATTY. 

In  the  fourth  month  my  boy  began  to  feel  over  surfaces  with  his  fingers,  — 
my  dress,  the  lounge  cover,  (114th  day),  the  wood  of  a  chair  back,  (117th  day). 

In  the  eleventh  month,  having  his  face  washed,  nose  wiped,  or  nails  cleaned, 
was  excessively  disagreeable  to  him. 

In  the  eighth  month,  236th  day,  he  bumped  his  head,  and  at  once  put  his 
hand  to  it  —  to  the  spot  of  the  injury,  his  nurse,  who  stood  nearer  him  than  I, 
told  me  —  and  rubbed  it.  In  the  eleventh  month  once  he  hurt  his  hand  in 
creeping,  and  cried  ;  and  when  I  picked  him  up  he  held  the  injured  hand  in 
the  other,  as  if  to  show  me  where  he  was  hurt.  A  few  days  earlier,  however, 
at  the  beginning  of  the  same  month,  having  received  a  bump  over  which  he 
cried  vigorously,  he  called  attention  to  it  by  pointing  his  finger  not  to  the  spot 
hurt,  but  to  the  place  on  the  wall  where  he  had  bumped  it. 

A  little  bruise  over  his  eye,  received  in  a  fall  from  the  lounge  on  the  227th 
day  (eighth  month)  seemed  to  remain  sore  for  some  time,  judging  by  his 
behavior  when  I  touched  it.  But  mental  diversion  easily  superseded  physical 
pain:  in  the  eleventh  month,  e.  g.,  a  visit  to  the  kittens  would  stop  the  hardest 
cry  over  a  bump,  etc. 

In  the  second  month  the  baby  cried  vigorously  when  the  water  of  his  bath 
was  too  hot. 

In  the  ninth  month,  255th  day,  as  I  was  giving  him  a  spoonful  of  water,  a 
drop  fell  on  his  hand,  and  he  immediately  looked  down  to  see  what  it  was. 


TASTE. 

i.  Sensibility. 

No  early  experiments  in  taste  were  tried  with  the  baby.  On  the 
2Sth  clay  a  little  sugar  was  given  her  to  stop  hiccoughs,  and  she 
seemed  to  like  it;  on  the  43d  day  it  was  received  withra  grimace  but 
swallowed  readily;  and,  rather  oddly,  water  was  received  in  the  same 
manner.  This  seems  to  have  been  the  same  grimace,  suggestive  of 
nauseous  bitterness,  that  Preyer  and  others  have  noted  as  common 
with  babies  on  experiencing  a  novel  taste.  During  the  fifth  and 
sixth  months  the  baby's  grandmother  several  times  gave  her  a  small 
taste  of  something  novel,  —  not  enough  to  swallow,  but  merely  to  be 
tasted  on  the  tongue,  —  but  the  grimace  did  not  appear  again  for 
several  weeks :  for  instance,  on  the  1 27th  day,  when  she  was  allowed 
to  suck  a  bit  of  apple,  the  baby's  face  showed  only  pleased  surprise, 
and  she  sat  perfectly  still  and  sucked  till  the  morsel  was  taken  away. 
On  the  173d  day,  however,  the  grimace  reappeared  at  a  taste  of 
meat-soup;  and  thereafter  to  the  end  of  the  seventh  month  it  was 
common.  It  became  rare  in  the  eighth,  and  I  noticed  it  but  twice 
in  the  ninth,  and  once  in  the  tenth;  never  thereafter.1 

As  has  been  observed  by  others,  this  grimace  had  no  connection 
with  repugnance  to  the  taste,  and  was  sometimes  followed  by  signs 
of  liking  and  desire:  thus  at  the  end  of  the  sixth  and  beginning  of 
the  seventh  months,  orange  juice  was  sucked  with  eagerness,  desired 
when  the  fruit  was  seen,  and  mourned  for  when  taken  away,  yet  was 
grimaced  over  extravagantly.  Nor  did  there  seem  to  be  any  rule 
as  to  the  kind  of  taste  that  excited  it.  It  was  perhaps  more  com- 
mon when  fruit  juice  was  tasted,  but  it  failed  to  appear  (seventh 
month,  209th  day)  at  the  first  taste  of  loquat,  and  in  several  other 
cases;  while  on  the  other  hand  it  did  appear  for  the  first  taste  of 
soup  and  of  broth  (late  in  the  sixth  month),  and  about  the  same 
time  a  piece  of  bread-crust  was  chewed   and  licked  with  grimaces. 

'Mrs.  Beatty's  boy  at  eight  months,  on  trying  a  new  taste  (milk  and 
scalded  bread),  shuddered  after  each  spoonful,  though  he  took  the  food  with 
out  resistance. 

(160) 


shins. i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  161 

The  two  instances  noted  in  the  ninth  month  were  caused  by  clear 
lemon  juice,  a  sufficiently  startling  taste,  but  the  one  instance  in 
the  tenth  month  by  baked  potato.  Nor  was  it.  even  associated  reg- 
ularly with  novelty :  thus  peach  juice  and  strawberry  juice  did  not 
excite  it  the  first  time  tasted,  but  the  second;  soup  and  bread-crust 
excited  it  the  first  time,  but  never  again;  sugar  repeatedly;  orange 
juice  always  for  weeks. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  fifth  month,  141st  day.  when  first  the 
baby  was  given  food  in  a  bottle,  she  made  much  objection  to  it,  we 
thought  because  the  nipple  was  of  fresh  rubber,  as  the  same  food 
had  been  readily  taken  for  three  days  with  a  spoon.  Early  in  the 
sixth  month,  154th  day,  she  several  times  carried  a  bright  celluloid 
ball,  which  had  a  strong  celluloid  taste  and  smell,  to  her  mouth, 
and  each  time  dropped  it  at  once  in  a  marked  manner.  Unless 
these  were  instances,  she  never  in  the  first  year  showed  any  percep- 
tion of  disagreeable  tastes.  She  was  sometimes  averse  to  taking 
food,  —  in  the  eighth  month,  e.  g.,  she  would  drop  her  jaw  to  let  a 
mouthful  of  milk  trickle  out;  but  this  was  due  to  want  of  appetite 
rather  than  to  distaste.  She  was  not,  it  is  true,  tested  with  dis- 
agreeable tastes ;  but  several  things  were  incidentally  tasted  that  she 
might  have  been  expected  to  find  disagreeable,  and  no  repugnance 
was  shown.  Clear  salt  she  did  not  notice  at  all  (eighth  month, 
228th  day);  sweet  oil  she  took  without  objection  (236th  and  237th 
day);  lemon  she  seemed  rather  to  like  (ninth  month,  255th  and  260th 
days);  at  ten  months  old  (304th  day)  she  touched  her  tongue  to  a 
spoon  that  had  contained  bitters,  and  showed  no  dislike;  in  the 
eleventh  month  she  liked  to  put  clear  dirt  into  her  mouth;  in  the 
last  week  of  the  year  (361st  day)  she  took  pleasure  in  mouthing  a 
green  orange,  though  she  could  hardly  have  failed  to  taste  the  pun- 
gent oil  of  the  skin.  These  things  seem  to  suggest  that  her  sense 
of  taste  was  dull,  but  the  great  joy  she  showed  in  certain  tastes  and 
her  distinct  preferences  among  them  contradict  the  inference.  Ot 
these  preferences  I  shall  speak  more  in  detail  below. 

In  the  second  year  also  there  was  little  outside  these  strong 
likings  to  show  any  keenness  of  taste.  In  the  thirteenth  month, 
she  seemed  to  like  any  novelty  well  enough.  In  the  fourteenth, 
she  sputtered  out  soap,  but  did  not  show  much  displeasure.     The 


162  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

first  time  she  commented  on  any  taste  with  disapproval  was  in  the 
eighteenth  month,  when  she  chewed  the  hulls  of  some  pecan  nuts) 
which  have  a  disagreeable  strong  flavor  much  like  that  of  walnut 
hulls.  She  had  been  told  that  she  would  not  like  shaddocks,  as  they 
were  sour;  and  when  I  took  the  pecan  hulls  from  her  mouth  she 
observed,  "  Shou ';  "  yet  she  was  interested  in  the  taste  and  very  will- 
ingto  try  more  of  it.  The  next  day  while  eating  a  cooky  she  saw  and 
desired  an  orange,  and  I  gave  it  to  her.  She  proceeded  to  eat  them 
alternately,  apparently  not  finding  that  the  cooky  made  the  orange 
sour  in  the  least.  In  this  eighteenth  month,  too,  the  old  wish  to  put 
dirt  into  her  mouth  recurred  quite  persistently.  In  the  nineteenth 
month  she  objected  to  a  medicine  when  first  tasted  that  it  was 
"hot,"  then  that  it  was  "sour"  (a  cough  syrup,  probably  with  a 
little  wild  cherry  flavor),  and  would  drop  her  jaw  and  let  it  trickle 
out.  A  small  bribe  of  hoarhound  candy  easily  overcame  her  objec- 
tion, and  she  took  the  medicine  cheerfully  thereafter,  and  seemed  to 
lose  all  distaste  for  it.  In  the  twentieth  month  she  did  not  like  the 
taste  of  an  oldish  mango,  and  said  it  was  "sour."  Early  in  the 
twenty-fourth  month  her  mother  noted  that  she  said  of  something 
she  did  not  like,  "That  nasty." 

In  spite  of  these  slight  exceptions,  it  seemed  to  me  until  about 
the  middle  of  the  third  year  that  the  child  liked  all  tastes  well 
enough,  though  some  she  liked  much  more  than  others.  At  the 
beginning  of  the  thirty-second  month  I  note  that  this  omnivorous- 
ness  is  now  past  and  there  are  many  things  she  refuses.  In  the 
thirty-fifth  month  I  gave  her  some  dill  to  taste:  she  said,  "I  don't 
like  it;  it's  medicine."  Her  experience  with  medicine  had  been  so 
very  slight  that  I  was  surprised,  and  asked  what  medicine  she  had 
had  that  she  didn't  like.  She  said  some  medicine  of  her  uncle's,  — 
some  white  medicine.  — Who  gave  it  to  her?  —  "I  got  it  myself."  — 
On  inquiry  I  found  she  had  got  hold  of  a  pill  and  licked  the  coat- 
ing off  Fennel  she  liked  better:  she  swallowed  it  and  said,  "I  like 
that  down  in  my  stomach." 

By  the  time  she  was  three  years  old,  she  became  very  shy  of 
trying  new  tastes,  and  at  present,  in  the  fourth  year,  seems  to  expect 
that  a  novelty  will  be  disagreeable  and  refuses  it, — sometimes  even 
when  we  assure  her  it  is  good;  she  seems  to  be  guided  a  good  deal 
in  this  by  its  appearance.  Earlier,  she  was  always  ready  to  try  any 
taste. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  163 

In  the  first  year,  and  to  a  much  less  extent  in  the  second,  novelty 
seemed  to  be  a  main  element  of  pleasure  in  taste  sensations.  Thus 
when  the  baby  was  first  allowed,  in  the  eighth  month  (216th  day), 
to  eat  a  little  meat-soup  and  cream,  which  she  had  barely  tasted 
before,  she  called  for  it  with  eager  cries  and  made  sounds  of  delight 
and  content  at  each  spoonful,  looking  up  at  her  father  and  smiling; 
and  when  at  just  nine  months  old  she  again  had  this  food,  she 
showed  similar  joy:  but  it  was  now  continued  regularly,  and  within 
six  weeks  she  no  longer  cared  for  it,  and  has  never  been  especially 
desirous  of  it  since.  At  eight  months  she  took  great  pleasure  in 
chewing  a  bread-crust  —  clung  to  it  and  would  not  be  parted  —  and  a 
few  days  later  was  consoled  when  bitterly  grieving  for  a  fruit  by  a 
crust;  but  after  tasting  a  pretzel  some  two  weeks  later  ceased  to 
care  much  for  crusts,  and  cared  still  less  for  them  in  the  next  month, 
after  trying  graham  flake  crackers.  After  about  a  month  more, by  the 
middle  of  the  eleventh  month,  she  tired  of  the  crackers  and  returned 
to  bread  and  toast  crusts.  During  the  ninth  and  tenth  months 
especially  her  appetite  was  not  good,  and  any  change,  as  from  oat- 
meal to  wheatina,  from  this  to  germea,  and  back  again,  seemed  to 
wake  a  new  pleasure  in  her  food,  and  also  to  make  it  agree  with  her 
better.  Her  first  real  considerable  pleasure  in  eating  came  with  her 
adoption  of  varied  diet  earlier  than  is  usually  advised.  But  other 
considerations  besides  taste  doubtless  entered  into  all  this. 

In  one  instance  there  was  a  reversal  of  the  rule  that  articles  of 
food  were  liked  best  while  new  to  her.  When  sweet  potato  was 
first  offered,  at  the  end  of  the  eleventh  month,  and  for  nearly  a 
month,  the  baby  cared  little  for  it;  then  became  extremely  fond  of 
it,  and  for  about  a  month  more  preferred  it  to  anything  else,  and 
would  squeal  and  clamor  for  it;  in  the  fourteenth  month  she  became 
indifferent  to  it  and  preferred  Irish  potato. 

2.  Special  Preferences. 

A  good  deal  has  been  indicated  above  as  to  the  child's  discrim- 
inations in  taste.  Her  principal  one,  however,  was  in  the  way  of 
preferences  in  food:  and  while  appetite  and  digestion  as  well  as 
taste  doubtless  entered  into  these  preferences,  and  idiosyncrasies  of 
no  general  interest  affected  them,  it  seems  necessary  to  any  full 
record  of  the  development  of  the  sense  that  I  give  some  account  of 
them  here. 


164  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

The  milk  diet  of  the  early  months  seems  never  to  have  given 
much  pleasure  to  the  sense  of  taste,  and  to  have  been  desired  for 
the  satisfaction  of  hunger  only.  Cow's  milk  or  diluted  cream,  with 
which  after  the  fifth  month  the  mother's  milk  was  supplemented, 
was  still  less  liked,  though  this  seemed  largely  due  to  dislike  of 
being  fed  with  a  bottle  or  spoon.  After  milk  became  but  one  article 
in  a  varied  diet,  by  the  end  of  the  first  year,  the  child  liked  it  well 
enough  but  never  showed  especial  pleasure  in  it,  and  at  times  ever 
since  has  refused  it  persistently,  wishing  to  drink  water  instead. 
(Her  craving  for  water  at  times  through  the  second  and  third  years 
has  been  remarkable;  she  would  ask  for  it  over  and  over  at  meals 
and  between  meals,  —  three  times  in  a  half  hour  I  noticed  once, 
when  she  was  not  in  the  least  feverish,  —  and  drank  really  large 
quantities.) 

The  gruels  and  porridges  with  which  the  milk  diet  was  varied 
in  the  ninth  month  were  usually,  as  noted  above,  much  liked  when 
first  tried,  but  had  to  be  changed  often  to  keep  up  the  child's  appe- 
tite at  all.  From  the  eleventh  month  through  the  second  year 
various  grain  foods  (including  bread  and  crackers),  with  Irish  and 
sweet  potato,  formed  the  principal  part  of  her  diet,  and  in  the  third 
year  these  foods  and  meat;  and  at  times  she  showed  much  pleasure 
in  them,  while  at  times  she  was  indifferent  to  them.  One  or  another 
article  would  be  favored  for  a  time  in  a  marked  degree.  Vegeta- 
bles  other  than  Dotato  were  rarely  liked;  but  celery  was  passionately 
desired 

Sweetness  did  not  seem  at  first  to  please  much.  At  the  first 
tastes  of  clear  sugar,  on  the  28th  and  43d  days,  no  decided  sign  of 
liking  was  given.  At  four  months  old  the  baby  was  fond  of  suck- 
ing a  rattle  handle  that  was  made  of  orris  root.  In  the  fifth  and 
sixth  months  the  mother  thought  milk  was  better  liked  when 
unsweetened.  In  the  whole  of  the  first  year  sugar  is  never  men- 
tioned among  the  things  that  gave  noticeable  pleasure.  The  day 
the  child  was  fifteen  months  old  she  started  to  climb  the  stairs  to 
me,  calling  me.  "  But  you  have  the  hiccoughs,"  her  grandmother 
said.  She  knew  this  meant  a  bit  of  candy  and  looked  back  toward 
it,  then  upstairs,  and  pushed  on  to  me.  Once  upstairs  she  would 
not  take  any  hint  that  she  should  go  down  and  get  the  candy. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  165 

After  this  she  became  much  fonder  of  candy,  and  begged  for  it 
when  it  was  about.  Cookies,  dates,  figs,  sugar  on  her  rice,  etc.,  are 
hereafter  mentioned  among  things  she  liked,  and  in  general  sweets 
are  several  times  in  my  notes  of  the  second  and  third  year  men- 
tioned as  among  her  very  greatest  desires. 

Fruit  was  earlier  and  on  the  whole  better  liked  than  pure  sweets.1 
The  first  distinct  pleasure  the  baby  ever  showed  in  a  taste  was 
when  she  first  tasted  fruit  juice  (127th  day,  see  p.  160).  Perhaps  once 
or  twice  a  month  in  the  next  three  months  she  was  given  a  mere 
taste  of  some  fruit  juice,  and  it  was  always  liked.  Near  the  end  of 
the  tenth  month  she  was  allowed  to  suck  fruit  through  a  thin  cloth 
every  few  days,  and  by  the  end  of  the  year  almost  daily ;  baked 
apple  or  pear  or  stewed  apple  sauce  was  also  added  to  her  meals. 
This  is  contrary  to  the  usual  rules  for  babies'  diet,  but  was  approved 
by  the  doctor  consulted,  and  seemed  under  the  closest  watching  to 
have  only  good  effects;  the  fruit  was  of  course  fresh  from  the  trees 
and  most  carefully  chosen.  It  was  always  liked:  most  new  tastes 
after  the  eighth  month  were  tried  with  grave  interest,  but  fruits 
with  laughter  and  demonstrations  of  joy;  and  even  after  the  taste 
ceased  to  be  novel  the  joy  would  often  be  shown,  less  excitedly. 
For  example,  in  the  eleventh  month  the  child  would  suck  her  peach 
with  a  happy,  serene  expression,  looking  about  at  us  from  time  to 
time  and  smiling,  or  calling  to  attract  our  notice  to  her  pleasure. 
At  times  the  enjoyment  of  some  other  article  of  food  would  equal 
that  of  fruit,  but  never  for  long.  At  various  times  in  the  second  or 
third  year,  fruit  and  going  outdoors  are  mentioned  as  the  child's 
two  great  and  dominant  objects  of  desire,  though  fruit  held  the 
second  place.  In  the  thirteenth  month,  e.  g.,  when  she  knew  she 
was  to  have  a  piece  of  watermelon  to  suck  or  that  she  was  to 
go  outdoors,  she  would  break  into  laughter  and  cries  of  approval, 
which  would  increase  as  she  saw  the  preparation,  and  would  be 
accompanied  by  lively  springing  motions.  By  the  third  year  she 
was  allowed  to  eat  fruit  quite  freely,  sometimes  two  or  three  oranges 
a  day. 

She  preferred  fruits  with  a  somewhat  lively  and  characteristic 

1  In  modification  of  the  evidence  my  notes  seem  to  give  of  this,  it  should  be 
said  that  fruit  was  so  much  more  freely  allowed  the  child  than  sweets,  that  she 
got  into  the  habit  of  asking  and  expecting  it  more. 


1 66  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

taste,  even  an  acid  one,  to  those  of  a  flat  sweetness.  As  early  as 
the  ninth  month,  when  I  twice  let  her  touch  her  tongue  to  a  piece 
of  lemon  (255th  and  260th  days)  she  reached  her  hands  for  more; 
in  the  eighteenth  month  she  ate  a  piece  of  lemon  as  big  as  the  end 
of  my  thumb  and  to  my  astonishment  asked  for  more;  in  the 
twenty-second  month  she  sucked  a  lemon  and  said,  "Good!"  and 
from  her  first  experience  of  it,  in  the  twenty-sixth  month,  she  has 
been  fond  of  lemonade,  which  she  never  cared  to  have  highly 
sweetened.  Oranges,  loquats,  peaches,  plums,  and  raspberries  were 
better  liked  than  apples  or  pears,  and  for  the  sweet  but  tasteless 
Japanese  persimmon  she  cared  little. 

The  only  cases  of  memory  in  taste  that  I  recorded  were  in  con- 
nection with  fruit.  In  the  eleventh  month,  309th  day,  fully  a  month 
after  the  season  of  loquats,  the  baby  discovered  a  bunch  of  half- 
dried  ones  (the  fruit  dries  up,  like  raisins,  instead  of  decaying)  on 
the  tree;  she  pointed  to  them  squealing  and  was  intensely  eager  to 
get  them,  and  absorbed  in  joy  and  expectation  while  I  peeled  them 
for  her.  In  the  fifteenth  month,  434th  day,  at  least  six  weeks  after 
watermelons  were  gone,  she  gave  a  shout  as  if  in  recognition  at  a 
picture  of  a  watermelon,  and  when  someone  told  her  to  "eat  it"  she 
fell  in  at  once  with  the  pretense.  Within  the  next  week  I  was  told 
she  made  a  noise  of  sucking  at  sight  of  a  picture  of  a  peach,  though 
peaches  had  disappeared  for  two  months  or  more. 

It  appears  a  generalization  of  the  idea  of  fruit  taste  that  after 
becoming  acquainted  with  peaches  and  loquats  the  baby  looked 
with  cries  of  desire  (tenth  month,  277th  day)  at  apricots,  which  she 
had  not  tasted.  By  the  end  of  the  year  she  was  not  slow  to  recog- 
nize any  new  kind  of  fruit  as  fruit,  after  a  period  of  interest  and 
experiment  in  everything  round  and  fruitlike. 

I  have  spoken  above  of  the  baby's  liking  for  meat  when  first 
tasted,  in  the  form  of  soup,  and  her  later  tiring  of  it  (p.  163.)  In  the 
eleventh  month  she  showed  so  persistent  lack  of  appetite  with  a 
milk  and  grain  diet  that  on  the  last  day  of  the  month,  at  our  doc- 
tor's advice,  a  bit  of  rare  broiled  steak  was  given  her  to  suck ;  and 
this  was  repeated  occasionally  in  the  twelfth  month.  She  did  not 
care  especially  for  it,  till  she  managed  to  bite  off  and  swallow  some 
of  it;  then  she  was  delighted;  and  as  it  digested  perfectly  well,  and 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  167 

her  appetite  for  other  food  revived  when  a  little  meat  was  given  with 
it  every  few  days,  it  was  continued,  and  the  allowance  was  grad- 
ually increased  during  the  second  year,  till  in  the  third  it  was  part 
of  her  regular  food.  During  most  of  the  second  year  her  enjoy- 
ment of  it  was  great,  and  it  ranked  with  sweets  as  her  greatest 
desire  in  food,  after  fruit;  at  times  it  was  as  much  desired  and  en- 
joyed as  fruit.  In  the  fifteenth  month  she  would  cry  out  for  it  per- 
sistently as  soon  as  the  platter  appeared.  After  the  twentieth  month 
her  great  preference  for  it  declined  on  the  whole,  though  with  reviv- 
als, and  it  is  well  liked  still. 

In  the  sixth  month  and  in  the  eighth  month  salt  put  into  the  baby's 
mouth  did  not  seem  to  be  tasted  at  all;  but  in  the  ninth  month  she 
was  very  fond  of  pretzels,  apparently  for  the  sake  of  the  salt,  which 
she  would  lick  and  suck  off;  and  in  the  second  year  she  became 
exceedingly  fond  of  clear  salt,  —  throughout  the  fifteenth  month 
she  would  beg  and  clamor  for  it,  and  was  fonder  of  it  than  of 
candy.     She  likes  her  food  pretty  well  salted  still. 

Several  foods  containing  oil  have  been  liked  from  the  first  taste, 
—  chiefly  butter  from  the  beginning  of  the  second  year,  olives  from 
the  twenty-second  month,  and  nuts  from  the  twenty-sixth  month. 
Nuts  especially  were  greatly  desired,  though  the  child  was  rarely 
allowed  to  have  one  till  late  in  the  third  year.  In  the  thirty-fifth 
month  I  gave  her  a  bit  of  green  almond.  "  I  like  it,"  she  said.  "  I 
like  it  all  the  way  down  to  my  stomach."  Chocolate  always  gave 
delight.  Japanese  ginger  has  been  consistently  liked  from  the  first 
taste,  at  eighteen  months  old.  In  all  her  tastes  that  seem  unusual, 
as  the  love  for  salt,  olives,  lemon,  ginger,  the  distaste  for  milk,  the 
child  reproduces  closely  those  of  either  father  or  mother. 

3.  Interest. 

Taste  at  no  time  played  as  large  a  part  among  the  child's  inter- 
ests as  I  had  expected.  In  the  first  eight  months,  which  were  filled 
with  a  multitude  of  the  most  lively  and  absorbing  pleasures  from 
the  higher  senses,  from  muscular  activity,  and  from  mental  occupa- 
tion, food  practically  afforded  no  interest  and  gave  no  pleasure 
beyond  the  negative  one  of  allaying  the  discomfort  of  hunger. 
Before  the  second  month  sight  had  excited  livelier  demonstrations 
than  taste  ever  did  before  the  eighth.     Although  in  the  first  half 


1 68  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

year  the  baby  was  often  keenly  hungry  and  demanded  food  impera- 
tively, as  soon  as  the  first  edge  of  hunger  was  off  she  cared  little  to 
continue  nursing,  and  wished  to  sit  up,  look  about,  and  play;  an)' 
sight  or  sound  would  divert  her.  From  the  eighth  month  demon- 
strations of  pleasure  in  food  appeared:  e.  g.,  in  the  tenth  month, 
304th  day,  when  the  mush  came  on  the  table  she  pointed  with  laugh- 
ter and  clamor  of  eagerness ;  in  the  eleventh,  3 1 5th  day,  she  greeted 
her  mother  and  the  a  bowl  of  wheatina  with  laughter  and  motions 
of  joy,  and  scrambled  down  from  my  lap  to  get  to  her.  At  other 
times  in  these  latter  months  of  the  first  year  even  the  best  liked 
food  was  treated  with  indifference,  and  she  preferred  playing  about 
to  eating.  Nor  did  the  greatest  pleasure  ever  called  out  by  taste 
in  this  six  months  equal  the  joy  that  was  habitual  over  the  dog, 
the  kittens,  going  outdoors,  etc. 

With  the  second  year,  interest  and  pleasure  in  food  increased,  and 
at  times  seemed  to  pass  all  others  except  that  in  going  outdoors, 
and  for  days  once  or  twice  to  rival  even  that;  after  the  child  was 
twenty-one  months  old,  however,  it  was  rare  that  she  was  willing 
to  leave  her  outdoor  play  to  get  her  meals.  Such  demonstrations 
of  interest  in  food  as  laughing  aloud  when  she  saw  it,  or  pointing 
to  her  high-chair  and  begging  before  breakfast  was  ready,  were 
quite  common  in  this  year;  in  the  nineteenth  month  once  as  the 
meat  was  removed  she  began  to  shout,  "Ea'  wi'!  ea'  wi'!"  —  now  we 
shall  eat  rice;  in  the  twentieth,  seeing  the  baker's  wagon  on  the 
road,  she  cried,  "  Brown  bread !  brown  bread ! "  till  she  got  it ;  so 
other  incidents  might  be  added.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  thirty -sixth 
month,  when  she  was  much  occupied  in  giving  accounts  of  some 
imaginary  parrots,  the  things  they  ate  had  a  large  place  in  her  inter- 
est. In  the  last  week  of  this  month  she  saw  a  circus  for  the  first 
time,  and  the  next  day  the  lemonade  and  popcorn  she  had  there 
proved  to  be  about  as  important  in  her  recollections  as  the  elephant. 

In  some  cases  other  interests,  usually  keen,  were  distinctly  sub- 
ordinated by  taste.  Thus  in  the  seventeenth  month  she  was  once 
given  a  piece  of  candy  just  before  my  return  from  the  city,  and  as 
soon  as  I  came  in  she  was  anxious  to  tell  me  about  it,  saying,  "Ca'! 
ca'!"  pointing  to  her  mouth  and  showing  the  bit  left  in  her  fingers, 
quite  oblivious  of  her  usual  lively  interest  in  my  return  and  the  dis- 
posal of  my  wraps.     Late  in  the  twenty-first  month,  missing  her 


shinn]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  169 

mother  one  day,  she  turned  from  all  her  favorite  diversions  to  ask 
pitifully,  "See  mamma!"  but  finally  the  suggestion  of  a  loquat  won, 
and  she  went  out  quite  happily  to  get  it,  —  though  this  may  have 
been  in  part  because  it  involved  going  outdoors.  In  the  first  week 
of  the  twenty-second  month  I  went  to  seethe  child,  who  was  camp- 
ing in  the  woods,  and  asked,  "What  do  you  suppose  grandma  sent 
you?"  —  "Candy?"— "No,  not  candy."  —  "Beefsteak?" —  "No." 
—  "Su  to  ea'?"  (something  to  eat)  — as  if  nothing  else  could  be  of 
interest.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  thirty-sixth  month,  asked,  "What 
would  you  like  to  do  on  your  birthday?"  she  answered,  "I  would 
like  to  have  some  candy  come." 

The  subordination  of  taste  by  other  interests  was  more  common, 
however.  The  child's  unwillingness  in  the  fourteenth  month  to  give 
up  a  visit  upstairs  for  the  sake  of  a  piece  of  candy  has  been  men- 
tioned (p.  164  ).  Early  in  the  sixteenth  month  she  begged  for  a 
book  while  eating  a  cooky,  and  was  told  she  must  finish  her  cooky 
first,  for  her  hands  were  dirty.  She  lifted  her  hand  and  inspected  it, 
then  held  out  the  cooky  and  intimated  that  she  wished  it  put  away 
on  the  mantel  while  she  went  to  get  her  hands  washed,  then  came 
back  and  called  again  for  the  "boo'."  So  again  in  the  latter  part  of 
the  eighteenth  month,  I  said  to  put  her  off  when  she  was  begging  to 
go  outdoors,  "  Why,  you  have  your  cooky  to  eat."  —  '"Way,"  she 
answered,  offering  the  cooky, — put  it  away  and  let  me  go.  In  the 
twenty-first  month,  wishing  to  take  her  into  the  house,  I  suggested 
that  she  should  go  indoors  and  have  a  cracker.  She  answered, 
"  That  cracker  go  outdoors  too  ?"  and  by  and  by  went  to  her  mother 
and  urged  the  suggestion  over  and  over  ;  she  evidently  wanted  the 
cracker,  but  not  unless  it  might  "go  outdoors  too."  Later  in  the 
same  month,  when  she  had  pinched  her  finger  and  was  asked 
whether  she  would  go  outdoors  or  have  her  breakfast  for  consola- 
tion, she  sobbed,  "Outdoors,"  and  as  soon  as  she  was  fairly  out 
stopped  her  crying  and  profuse  kissing  of  her  finger,  and  said  with 
an  air  of  relief,  "  Feel  better  now."  The  next  day  I  could  not  coax 
her  to  desert  the  joy  of  climbing  up  and  down  the  stairs  for  the 
sake  of  her  dinner,  and  had  to  pick  her  up  and  bring  her  resisting 
to  the  table;  but  after  dinner,  when  she  had  started  up  the  stairs 
again  and  I  had  suggested  that  if  she  would  come  down  her  mother 
would  let  her  run  about  with  no  clothes  on,  she  instantly  came  back 
laughing  and  eager.     So  other  instances  might  be  given. 


170  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Desire  for  taste  sensations  was  at  all  times  noticeably  influenced 
by  habit,  and  controllable.  From  the  time  she  could  be  propped  with 
cushions  in  a  high-chair  she  sat  at  the  family  table,  and  she  never 
gave  trouble  by  demanding  the  things  she  saw  others  eating.  In 
the  second  year  she  sometimes  squealed  and  shouted  for  articles 
she  had  been  allowed  to  have ;  those  she  had  never  been  permitted 
she  saw  eaten  every  day  without  thinking  of  wishing  them.  For 
things  sometimes  given  her  and  sometimes  withheld  she  at  times 
asked  persistently,  but  as  a  rule  accepted  refusal  pleasantly.  In  the 
twentieth  and  twenty-first  months,  e.  g.,  she  would  ask  at  all  hours 
for  cherries,  loquats  or  berries, but  if  refused  would  say,  "Make 
Ruth  sick,"  and  think  no  more  of  it.  She  knew  where  they  all 
grew,  and  would  make  visits  to  the  trees,  pick  up  fallen  fruit,  and 
ask,  "  Good  ripe  cherry  ?  "  or  "  loquat ; "  and  if  told  it  was  green  01 
bad  would  throw  it  willingly  away.  She  would  punctiliously  ask 
if  she  might  pick  a  fruit,  or  eat  it  when  in  her  hand:  "Woo  ha'  li' 
pea'?"  (Ruth  have  a  little  peach?)  she  would  ask  (twenty-second 
month);  or,  "  Mamma,  do  you  sink  I  had  better  have  some  lokats  ?" 
(thirty-third  month).  In  the  thirty-fourth  month  I  gave  her  some 
candy  to  put  away  in  a  receptacle,  telling  her  she  must  not  eat  it; 
she  sat  in  my  lap  a  half  hour,  happy  and  interested  in  examining 
the  pieces  and  stowing  them  away  without  offering  to  eat  one,  only 
once  or  twice  touching  a  novel  kind  to  her  tongue  to  test.  It  was 
now  possible  to  leave  candy  in  the  room  accessible  to  her,  with  a 
fair  certainty  that  she  would  not  touch  it. 

There  were  marked  exceptions  to  this  ease  in  inhibition  of'  taste 
desires,  ■ — ■  rather  freakish  ones,  it  seemed  to  me,  than  due  to  any 
special  fondness  for  certain  articles;  thus  in  the  eighteenth  month, 
her  persistent  wish  to  put  dirt  in  her  mouth  when  digging,  could 
hardly  have  been  due  to  love  of  the  taste.  In  the  twenty- fifth  she 
once  suddenly  ran  to  her  father's  desk,  climbed  a  chair,  and  before 
she  could  be  reached  seized  a  piece  of  peppermint,  stuffed  it  into 
her  mouth,  and  hastily  swallowed  it  as  I  approached.  In  the  latter 
part  of  the  twenty-sixth  month,  seeing  her  mother  handling  some 
peaches,  she  urged  over  and  over,  "Have  own  self!"  and  when 
refused,  she  snatched  the  table  cover  and  jerked  it  to  bring  the  fruit 
within  reach;  when  her  hand  was  slapped  for  this  she  remarked 
mournfully,  "  Hurt  dear  little  hand!"  —  then  in  a  few  seconds  was 


i  in.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  \-/i 

cheerfully  interested  in  seeing  the  peaches  put  into  a  pail  to  take  to 
her  aunt,  and  started  off  for  the  buggy  with  them,  calling,  "  Here 
go  peach  for  Aunt  May,"  without  showing  the  least  desire  for  them 
herself.  In  the  twenty-fourth  month  she  was  for  a  few  days  deter- 
mined to  snatch  the  prunes  that  were  spread  all  about  on  drying 
frames;  she  would  clutch  a  handful  and  double  herself  over  them, 
so  that  they  would  have  to  be  taken  from  her  by  force ;  but  she 
became  in  a  few  days  very  obedient  to  me  about  them  and  would 
play  with  piles  of  them  without  thinking  of  putting  one  into  her 
mouth.  In  the  next  month  she  was  still  more  persistent  about 
seizing  almonds ;  but  after  the  first,  the  disobedience  was  a  matter 
of  occasional  sudden  freak;  thus  in  the  twenty-fifth  month  she 
would  amuse  herself  by  cracking  almonds  for  many  minutes  with- 
out offering  to  touch  one,  but  now  and  then  would  suddenly  dive 
under  the  table  with  a  kernel  and  lie  on  her  face  to  conceal  it,  or 
slip  one  into  her  mouth  and  cover  her  face  with  her  hands.  This 
behavior  was  due  to  someone's  having  irresponsibly  given  her  prunes 
or  nuts  when  she  knew  they  were  forbidden;  and  after,  late  in  the 
third  year,  her  mother  "allowanced"  her  to  one  nut  a  day,  she 
became  absolutely  faithful  to  the  restriction,  and  could  be  trusted 
to  go  to  a  boxful  at  will,  select  her  nut,  and  wish  no  more  till 
next  day.  She  would  even  refuse  unauthorized  offers,  saying 
reprovingly,  "  I  must  not  have  that,"  or  "  I  have  had  my  nut  to-day." 
From  the  beginning  of  the  second  year  I  noticed  her  invariable 
willingness  to  wait  cheerfully  for  an  expected  dainty, —  while,  e.  g., 
(thirteenth  month),  a  bit  of  watermelon  was  tied  in  a  cloth  for  her 
to  suck,  and  she  herself  in  a  towel ;  or  while  others  were  waited  on 
at  table.  A  little  of  a  dainty  did  not  whet  desire  unless  she  had 
been  led  to  expect  more;  at  nineteen  months  she  would  finish  a 
date  or  lozenge,  then  thrust  her  hands  behind  her  with  a  quaint  and 
expressive  gesture  and  cry  cheerfully,  "Gogng!"  (gone),  and  ask 
for  no  more.  With  the  latter  half  of  the  third  year  I  began  to 
notice  her  invariable  willingness  to  share  her  candy,  bananas,  etc. ; 
she  would  offer,  even  urge,  the  last  piece,  look  at  the  empty  box 
and  say  cheerfully,  "All  gone,"  and  turn  to  some  other  interest. 
Indeed,  to  see  others  eating  a  favorite  food  was  often  desired  as  a 
substitute  for  eating  it  herself;  in  the  thirty-sixth  month,  e.  g-.,  when 
prunes  were  denied  her  she  would  beg  me  to  eat  them  then,  and 
take  great  pleasure  in  bringing  them  to  me. 


172  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

Much  of  all  this  was  temperament,  and  was  shown  in  other 
inhibitions  besides  those  of  taste  desires;  in  the  matter  of  picking 
flowers,  for  example.  Some  of  it,  however,  seems  due  to  the  effect 
of  systematic  restriction  in  keeping  the  interest  in  taste  sensations 
from  becoming  absorbing.  For  a  time  about  the  seventeenth  month, 
when  those  in  charge  had  slipped  into  some  laxity  in  letting  the 
child  have  fruit,  candy,  etc.,  unsystematically,  she  grew  exigent, 
thought  a  great  deal  more  about  eating,  and  seemed  to  experience 
more  desire  for  these  indulgences  than  before  or  since;  she  would 
tease  for  them  all  day,  and  one  afternoon  (when,  to  be  sure,  she  was 
fretful  from  other  causes)  she  several  times  threw  herself  on  the 
floor  and  cried  when  candy  was  refused.  I  have  little  doubt  that 
taste  might  easily  have  become,  by  more  attention,  a  much  more 
dominant  sensation;  or  had  the  child  not  been  well  supplied  with 
matter  of  interest  to  the  other  senses,  had  she  been  more  confined 
to  the  house  and  become  restless  for  occupation,  she  might  have 
been  thrown  back  more  upon  taste  interest. 

Of  any  influence  of  suggestion  on  taste,  I  saw  practically  no 
indication.  The  nearest  approach  to  it  was  that  the  same  food 
rejected  in  her  own  seat  would  be  a  few  minutes  later  eaten  with 
relish,  when  she  had  been  allowed  to  go  and  sit  in  her  grandmoth- 
er's lap,  —  the  regular  indulgence  at  the  close  of  a  meal  in  the  lat- 
ter part  of  the  second  year.     The  following  might  seem  an  instance: 

—  In  the  thirtieth  month  she  was  in  the  habit  of  leaving  her  crusts 
uneaten,  and  I  remarked  on  this  in  her  hearing,  saying  that  I  won- 
dered why  children  disliked  crusts,  for  the  taste  was  agreeable. 
The  child  took  up  a  rejected  crust,  ate  it,  and  called  my  attention, 

—  "Aunty,  I  did  eat  dat  good  c'ust."  For  at  least  a  week  after- 
ward I  noticed  that  she  daily  went  patiently  through  her  crusts.  I 
did  not  think,  however,  that  suggestion  had  influence  here;  either 
she  wished  to  please,  or  liked  the  crusts  better  on  acquaintance;  she 
has  not  rejected  them  consistently  since.  She  would  sometimes 
wish  to  try  a  taste  because  someone  else  thought  it  good ;  but  I 
never  knew  a  case  in  which  the  most  skillful  effort  could  affect  her 
opinion  of  it  after  she  had  tried  it. 

The  one  instance  in  which  I  knew  her  to  take  what  might  be 
called  an  experimental  interest  in  taste  was  in  the  thirty-fifth  month. 
I  had  noticed  that  she  was  often  very  unwilling  to  try  new  tastes, 


shinn.i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  173 

and  thought  it  well  to  correct  this  a  little.  I  found  that  a  very 
slight  suggestion  addressed  not  to  her  desire  of  taste  but  to  her 
curiosity  (as,  "You  may  try  how  that  tastes,  if  you  like")  was 
responded  to,  and  she  went  about  with  me  for  a  long  time  tasting 
various  things  in  the  garden,  interested,  and  always  willing  to  try. 
Of  course  I  took  precautions  against  her  being  led  by  the  incident 
to  any  dangerous  independent  investigation  of  the  sort,  and  she 
has  never  shown  the  least  disposition  to  make  such.  The  immense 
curiosity  concerning  things  to  be  seen,  heard,  or  felt  does  not  seem 
to  extend  to  the  realm  of  taste. 

Words  of  taste,  even  the  names  of  articles  of  food,  did  not 
appear  especially  early  in  her  vocabulary.  Milk,  cracker,  and  water 
were  among  some  forty-five  words  understood  by  her  at  eleven 
months,  and  a  number  more  were  learned  in  the  twelfth  month,  but 
her  curiosity  about  the  names  of  the  things  she  ate  was  out  of  all 
proportion  less  than  about  those  of  the  things  she  saw  and  handled. 
In  the  fifteenth  month  she  first  named  a  food  herself,  and  by  the 
end  of  the  sixteenth,  fourteen  names  of*  food  were  used,  out  of  a 
vocabulary  of  sixty-five  words.  There  seemed  little  relation  between 
their  order  of  appearance  and  her  preferences  in  taste. 

In  the  sixteenth  month  I  heard  the  child  saying,  "Goo',  goo'," 
and  asked  her,  "What  is  good?"  and  she  answered  by  pointing  into 
her  mouth.  After  this,  good  came  into  unmistakable  use,  and  up  to 
the  eighteenth  month  referred  exclusively  to  agreeable  taste;  after 
that  it  was  occasionally  used  in  other  senses,  as  of  a  good  (unbroken) 
pen,  or  of  her  own  conduct.  In  the  eighteenth  month  sour  appeared, 
meaning  any  disagreeable  taste;  this  was  used  for  several  months, 
but  lost  before  the  end  of  the  second  year,  "I  don't  like  it"  becom- 
ing the  usual  expression  of  distaste.  Her  calling  medicine  hot  I 
have  mentioned  (p.  162).  Nasty  was  once  used  of  a  bad  taste, 
(thirty-sixth  month)  but  never  really  taken  up. 

Of  adjectives,  two  of  sight,  dark  and  dirty,  and  one  of  dermal 
sensation,  wet,  preceded  the  first  adjective  of  taste,  good. 

Eat  appeared  in  the  sixteenth  month,  but  as  late  as  the  twenty- 
second  I  doubted  if  it  was  distinguished  from  biting,  or  merely  put- 
ting in  the  mouth;  she  once  even  used  the  word  of  kissing  (p.  139). 
Lunch,  used  in  the  nineteenth  month,  meant  any  meal,  perhaps  table 
food  in  general.  Taste  (the  neuter  verb,  "  taste  good")  appeared 
in  the  twenty-first  month. 


SMELL. 
i.   Sensibility. 

In  the  first  ten  months  I  saw  no  indication  of  the  existence  of  a 
sense  of  smell,  but  no  very  strong  odors  were  tried.  In  the  tenth 
month  the  baby's  grandmother  tried  a  good  deal  to  teach  her  to 
smell  at  flowers,  and  a  camphor  bottle  also  had  repeatedly  been  held 
to  her  nose;  at  just  ten  months  she  for  the  first  time  sniffed  when  a 
flower  or  bottle  was  offered,  and  I  thought  by  her  manner  that  she 
perceived  some  sensation.1  She  lost  the  trick  of  sniffing  in  a  few 
days,  however,  and  when  I  tried  to  reteach  it,  on  the  318th  day,  she 
confused  it  with  kissing,  and  sniffed  at  the  picture  she  had  been 
taught  to  kiss.  In  the  next  few  days  she  recovered  from  this  con 
fusion,  but  had  no  idea  of  the  purpose  of  the  act,  and  when  told  to 
"smell"  she  would  sniff  without  any  object  to  smell;  or  she  would 
sometimes  do  it  vigorously  for  fun.  The  330th  day  I  was  told  that 
she  associated  the  act  with  the  Catalonian  jasmine,  which  had  often- 
est  been  used  in  teaching  her,  and  would  sniff  when  she  passed  it; 
but  when  I  tried  to  get  her  really  to  smell  it  she  would  turn  away 
her  head,  or  put  the  offered  flower  into  her  mouth. 

Camphor,  however,  she  seemed  to  me  in  this  month  to  like 
to  inhale.  The  321st  day  I  put  some  on  her  upper  lip,  where 
she  must  inhale  it,  and  she  looked  at  me  with  a  puzzled  expression 
for  a  few  seconds.  In  the  thirteenth  month  I  thought  she  liked  to 
have  camphor  about  her. 

In  the  fourteenth  month  she  began  to  sniff  at  roses,  —  perhaps 
by  mere  association  of  the  act  with  flowers:  the  403d  day  she  bent 
to  smell  a  picture  of  a  rose,  and  in  the  last  week  of  the  month 
would  point  and  sniff  when  she  wished  to  be  lifted  up  to  smell  pic- 
tures of  roses  and  of  nasturtiums  on  the  wall.  Twice  in  this  month 
404th  and  417th  days,  she  laughed  aloud  when  held  down  to  smell 
flowers  in  the  dark,  and  I  thought  it  might  be  at  the  recognition  of 
their  odor;  but  for  the  next  three  months  I  saw  no  indication  but 

1  Mrs.  Beatty  never  saw  her  boy  sniff  at  a  flower  till  the  eleventh  month; 
but  she  was  told  of  his  doing  it  in  the  ninth. 

(■74) 


shins  i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  175 

this  of  anything  more  than  a  mere  association  of  the  act  of  sniffing 
with  flowers.  A  similar  association  with  bottles  was  sometimes 
shown.  Once  on  the  412th  day  she  smelled  of  my  watchguard 
again  and  again,  a  meaningless  act  so  far  as  I  could  see. 

On  the  463d  day  I  was  told  that  she  showed  pleasure  at  the  smell 
of  whale-oil  soap,  and  wished  to  smell  it  again. 

In  the  third  week  of  the  eighteenth  month  I  gave  her  an  orange 
blossom  to  smell,  —  the  first  time  she  had  seen  the  flower.  She 
called  it  lilac  at  once ;  she  could  have  had  no  reason  for  doing  so 
except  the  perception  that  both  were  fragrant.  She  knew  a  number 
of  flower  names  at  this  time,  and  usually  experienced  no  confusion 
between  them.  She  seemed  really  interested  in  the  flower,  and 
desired  one  every  time  we  went  near  the  trees,  and  would  carry  it 
about  in  her  hand  for  many  minutes;  yet  she  would  not  voluntarily 
smell  it.  In  a  day  or  two  she  called  a  verbena  a  lilac;  in  this  case 
there  was  no  decided  perfume  to  account  for  the  confusion,  but  there 
is  a  slight  resemblance  in  appearance ;  and  I  saw  her  smell  the 
flower,  as  if  to  see  if  it  really  were  lilac. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  month  occurred  unmistakable  evidence 
at  last  of  a  sense  of  smell,  and  power  of  discrimination  by  it,  and 
memory  connected  with  it.  The  mother  told  me  that  the  child, 
kissing  her  soon  after  she  had  eaten  a  wintergreen  lozenge,  had 
cried,  "Candy ! "  I  then  put  some  camphor  on  my  handkerchief 
(out  of  the  child's  sight,  of  course)  and  offered  it  to  her  to  smell. 
She  cried,  "Camphor!"  then,  "More!"  a'nd  ran  to  the  door  of  the 
closet  where  the  bottle  was  kept,  begging,  "Camphor!  camphor!" 
In  the  first  week  of  the  nineteenth  month  she  recognized  lilac  when 
thrust  into  the  bush  after  dark  to  smell,  but  it  was  moonlight,  and 
she  might  have  recognized  the  location.  In  the  same  week  I  gave 
her  a  sniff  at  an  orange  blossom  that  I  held  in  my  closed  hand,  and 
she  said  it  was  "lilac." 

Though  she  undoubtedly  could  now  recognize  a  smell,  she  still 
treated  smelling  as  a  mere  ceremony  associated  with  flowers.  She 
had  been  told  she  might  smell'  flowers  that  she  was  not  allowed  to 
pick,  and  in  the  latter  part  of  the  twentieth  month  it  became  a  reg- 
ular routine  to  go  and  smell  one  when  she  was  forbidden  to  pick  it. 
"Ruth  pick?"  she  would  say.  —  "No."  —  "Look?"  —  "Yes,  Ruth 
may  look."  —  "Smell?"  and   she  would  bend  over  and  sniff.     She 


176  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

carried  it  so  far  that  when  on  the  592c!  day  she  saw  some  files  of 
magazines  she  was  not  allowed  to  handle,  she  began,  "Ruth?  mag- 
azine?" —  "No,  Ruth  must  not  play  with  the  magazines."  —  "Look?" 

—  "Yes,  Ruth  may  look."  —  "  Smell?"  and  she  bent  over  and  sniffed 
punctiliously  and  loudly  at  one  after  another.  So  a  few  days  later, 
in  a  parlor  where  she  was  forbidden  to  touch  the  books,  she  would 
explain,  "  Look,"  and  "Smell,"  and  act  accordingly.  She  kept  up 
this  smelling  at  forbidden  flowers  —  nearly  all  odorless,  as  it  chanced 

—  for  a  month  or  more,  while  she  never  offered  to  smell  the  sweet- 
peas  she  was  daily  playing  with.  She  showed  no  sign  of  preferring 
fragrant  flowers. 

Her  father,  who  does  not  smoke  himself,  would  sometimes  come 
home  redolent  from  sitting  an  hour  in  the  smoking  car;  but  the 
child  never  seemed  to  notice  any  difference  when  he  took  her  in  his 
arms.  In  the  twenty-third  month  she  would  sometimes  say,  "That 
don't  smell  good,"  or  "don't  smell  nice,"  but  I  did  not  know  how 
far  it  was  a  mere  echo  of  comments  she  had  heard  others  make. 
Even  as  late  as  the  twenty-seventh  month,  when  she  would  say 
such  things,  —  e.  g.,  "  I  don't  like  smoke,  —  that  smells  bad,"  — 
I  thought  it  was  because  she  had  heard  someone  else  say  so ;  but 
her  mother  said  she  had  heard  such  an  expression  when  it  was 
plainly  an  original  opinion.  In  the  thirty-third  month,  her  mother 
told  me,  she  brought  a  piece  of  fennel  indoors,  then  smelling  it 
observed,  "This  does  n't  smell  nice  in  this  country;  it  smells  better 
outdoors."  (I  had  told  her  a  cousin  was  in  Germany,  but  most  of 
her  friends  were  "in  this  country.") 

2.  Interest. 

I  judge  that  interest  in  smell  was  so  slight  that  the  sense  seemed 
later  in  development  and  less  keen  than  it  really  was.  Once  only,  in 
the  last  week  of  the  twenty-seventh  month,  under  suggestion  from 
me,  the  child  became  very  much  interested  in  going  about  and  finding 
how  the  different  leaves  in  the  garden  smelled,  —  camphor,  lemon 
verbena,  orange,  sage,  etc.  Once,  too,  in  the  twenty-ninth  month, 
she  stopped  to  smell  a  flower,  then  called  to  me,  "Come  smell  this 
daffodil,"  —  a  flower,  however,  of  but  the  slightest  odor. 

Asked  in  the  twentieth  month,  "What  does  Ruth  do  with  her 
nose?"  she  answered  by  wrinkling  it  up  gayly  into  a  grimace  then 
frequent  with  her,  and  did  not  think  of  its  use  in  smelling  till  helped 
by  hints  about  the  flowers. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  177 

The  word  smell,  as  noted  above,  appeared  in  the  twentieth  month, 
but  only  to  mean  the  act  of  sniffing.  In  the  twenty-third  it  was 
used  in  the  neuter  sense,  "to  smell  good,"  but  it  is  unlikely  that  it 
was  used  intelligently  before  the  twenty-seventh;  so  too  of  the 
adjectives  good  and  nice  used  with  it.  Smell  bad  occurred  in  the 
twenty-third  month.  In  the  twenty-first  month  she  had  said  sweet 
after  smelling,  or  when  asked,  "  How  does  it  smell?"  but  I  did  not 
think  the  word  had  any  meaning  to  her. 


The  most  important  generalization  that  appears  to  me  on  com- 
pleting this  analysis  of  my  notes  on  the  development  of  the  sensesi 
is  one  contrary  to  accepted  opinion  and  to  my  own  expectation  in 
beginning  the  observations,  but  in  accord  with  my  general  impres- 
sion as  they  proceeded  — viz.,  that  the  higher  senses  led  from  the 
first  in  the  child's  psychological  activity.  So  far  from  finding  an 
early  dominance  of  taste  and  smell,  displaced  later  by  that  of  the 
senses  that  supply  more  mental  interest,  I  found  a  lively  attention 
to  sight  impressions  very  early,  slowly  overtaken  by  attention  to 
other  sensations  in  direct  rather  than  In  inverse  order  of  their  in- 
tellectual importance. 

Putting  out  of  account  the  organic  craving  of  hunger,  I  did 
not  see  evidence,  least  of  all  in  the  earliest  months,  that  taste,  and 
smell,  and  dermal  pain,  excited  keener  interest,  as  compared  with 
sight  or  hearing,  than  they  do  in  adults, —  possibly  not  so  keen. 
I  did  not  see  that  they  occupied  a  larger  proportion  of  the  total 
attention  given  to  pure  or  almost  pure  sensation  than  in  theaverage 
adult,  —  possibly  not  so  large 


178  University  of  California.  Vol.  i 

Note   to   Page  128. 

Though  I  have  not  thought  it  best  to  include  in  the  text  any  incidents  from 
the  fourth  year,  I  append  here  a  few  farther  notes  on  the  rude  improvising  of 
chants,  since  it  was  a  continuous  episode,  beginning  in  the  last  days  of  the 
third  year  and  running  on  for  weeks  into  the  fourth. 

During  these  weeks  —  the  whole  of  the  thirty-seventh  and  thirty-eighth 
months  —  the  child  continued  now  and  then  to  fit  her  chanting  to  half  metrical 
lines,  usually  made  in  the  same  form  as  those  on  p.  127,  "All  about  the  —  ," 
etc.  They  were  wholly  illogical,  but  perhaps  not  absolutely  meaningless  to  the 
child,  in  the  way  of  broken  suggestion  and  images.  One  that  I  caught  in  the 
thirty-eighth  month,  sung  as  the  child  danced  about,  was:  — 

"  The  gentle  town,  the  gentle  town,  the  gentle  lawn, 
Will  tin  //"  harm  to  us." 

The  only  time  that  the  lines  were  deliberately  made  was  late  in  the  thirty- 
seventh  month,  when  the  child  told  me,  among  other  stories  about  some  imag- 
inary chipmunks,  that  she  bought  them  a  book  "  in  my  city,"  and  the  book  had 
pictures  and  poems  in  it.    Then  she  chanted  loudly:  — 

"All  about  rein'  up  in  de  'ky, 
De  little  bad,     - 

"Dc  g'eenic  has  a  whitish  book. 

"  Dat's  a  poem." 

The  first  fragment  was  from  a  kindergarten  song:  — 
"  //  'ay  11  f>  in  the  sky, 
The  little  birds  fly." 

The   "greenie"  in  the  second  was  from  an  advertising  adaptation  of  Cox's 
brownies.     Other  fragments  followed,  so  pointless  that  I  could  not  impress  them 
on  my  memory.     Getting  a  pencil  and  paper,  I  asked  her  to  tell  me  some  of 
the  poems  again;  and  she  readily  chanted  the  following:  — 
"Kitty  has  a  lit-tell poem. 

"Kitty  lia^  a  lit-tell  jum- tick  [drum-stick']; 
Kitty  kimbs  [climbs]  on  de  jum- tick  andp'ays  ">i  hay  pi-yanno, 

"In  de  jum- tick  way  to  p'ay  on  hay  pi-yanno." 
"Dose  are s'orl  poems,"  she  said  in  a  tone  of  criticism.  —  "  Very  short,"  I 
said.     "  Can't  you  tell  me  a  longer  one  ?  "     But  she  was  now  interested  in  tell- 
ing me  about  kitty's  piano,  and  recited  no  more  poems. 

After  the  thirty-eighth  month,  though  the  child  now  and  then  threw  into  a 
chant  what  she  was  saying,  I  heard  no  more  of  the  "poems."  Thus  in  the 
forty-fourth  month,  as-  she  ran  past  my  window,  I  heard  her  call  in  a  singing 
tone  to  some  birds  that  flew  out  of  a  tree  near  by:  — 

"Fly, fly, fly,  hnds! 
I  'llget  a  laddei .  andgei  way  up  there  and  catchydu, 
li   vou  don't  fly,  fly,  fly!" 

This  is  evidently  quite  different  in  spirit  from  the  earlier  attempts;  it  is 
merely  an  adaptation  to  chanting  of  what  the  child  meant  to  say  in  any  case; 
not  a  "poem,"  or  "song,"  in  which  mening  was  entirely  subordinate. 

Milicent  Washburn  Shinn. 


Numbers  III  and   IV  of  the  Notes  on  tin-  Development  of  a  Child  are   published 
through  the  kind   liberality  ol   Mrs.  PHEBE  A.    HEARST. 


7th  month. — The  photograph  shows  that  traces  of  the  eaflv 
grasping  with  both  hands,  the  fingers  vaguely  spread  until  the 
object  was  touched,  were  still  to  be  seen.  See  pages  313,  315, 
,V9.  323. 


7th  mouth. — One  of  the  earliest  stages  in  the  development 
of  creeping,  the  mere  habit  of  various  positions  propped  up 
on  the  hands  and  knees,  is  well  shown  here.  See  pages  335, 
4<>7.  4M- 


9th  month.  — Standing  with  the  slight  support  of  the  door 
corner.     See  page  345. 


One  year  old. — The  ease  and  freedom  of  this  position  in 
standing  (a  spontaneous  one,  frequently  taken  at  the  time). 
will  give  some  idea  of  the  development  in  security  of  balance. 
I  have  no  satisfactory  photograph  of  the  standing  position 
entirely  unsupported,  which  was  completely  acquired  by  this 
date. 


SENSATIONS   OF   MUSCULAR  ACTIVITY,  MOTION, 
AND   POSITION. 

The  sensations  here  grouped,  (quite  various  in  their  physio- 
logical origin,  but  too  closely  interwoven  in  the  development  of  the 
child  to  be  separated  in  the  record,)  are  among  the  most  important 
of  the  first  and  second  year.  This  is  evident,  not  only  from  the 
remarkable  extent  to  which  the  pleasures  of  this  period  are  made 
up  of  motion,  active  and  passive,  but  also  from  the  numerous  and 
complex  muscular  adjustments  that  are  acquired,  impossible  with- 
out the  guidance  of  sensation.  Directing  the  eyes,  seizing  and 
handling  objects,  walking,  talking,  are  examples.  But  the  greater 
part  of  the  account  of  these  sensations  is  inseparably  involved  in 
that  of  the  movements,  and  cannot  well  be  given  here.  The  notes 
that  follow,  therefore,  are  but  the  incomplete  record  of  this  group 
of  sensations  that  can  be  detached  from  my  later  notes  on  move- 
ment, and  give  but  an  inadequate  impression  of  their  importance. 

i.  Muscular  Sensation. 

We  have  no  positive  evidence  of  the  presence  of  muscular  sen- 
sation until  the  appearance  of  voluntary  movements  requiring  its 
guidance;  still,  it  is  reasonable  to  suppose  that  it  accompanies 
movement  from  the  first;  and  the  earliest,  unregulated  movements 
may  be  of  considerable  service  in  developing  it  and  differentiating 
it  in  the  consciousness,  till  it  is  able  to  take  its  part  in  directing 
voluntary  movement.  Thus  sneezing  and  crying  from  the  first 
hour,  and  afterwards  sucking,  starting,  winking,  smiling,  hiccough- 
ing, yawning,  stretching,  panting,  and  varied  vocal  exercises,  gave 
quite  a  wide  experience  of  muscular  sensation  during  the  first  two 
months.  Still  more  efficient  in  this  direction  must  have  been  the 
aimless  movements  called  by  Preyer  "impulsive,"  since  these  were 
remarkably  profuse  and  continuous  during  the  first  weeks.  From 
the  first  hour  the  arms  moved  quite  freely,  say  three  or  four  inches: 
and  on  the  4th  day  the  nurse,  trying  to  trace  the  baby's  hand  on 
paper,  found  that  even  in  sleep  she  would  not  let  it  be  held  still, 
and  kept  the  fingers  sprawled.     On  the  5th  day  I  noticed  that  when 

13  (.79) 


IoO  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

she  was  awake  and  comfortable,  her  head  was  much  of  the  time 
moving  vaguely  from  side  to  side;  so  constantly,  indeed,  were  the 
head  and  eyes  moved  during  the  first  month,  that  so  late  as  the 
29th  day,  I  could  not,  in  repeated  trials,  tell  whether  the  eyes  really 
followed  a  moving  object  for  a  few  inches,  or  only  moved  with  it 
by  chance.  Violent  grimacing  was  also  frequent  (this,  however, 
not  entirely  without  peripheral  stimulus,  as  it  seemed  connected 
with  faint  sensations  of  discomfort);  and  arm  and  leg  movements 
were  practically  continuous  when  the  baby  was  wide  awake  and 
comfortable. 

These  irregular  movements  disappeared1  with  the  appearance  of 
more  voluntary  and  co-ordinated  ones,  as  if  inhibited  by  them 
Even  staring,  in  the  third  week,  tended  to  inhibit  the  grimacing 
and  the  aimless  movements  of  eyes  and  head;  and  with  the  acquire- 
ment of  fixation  (4th —  8th  weeks:  see  Sight,  pp.  14,  15),  and  of 
ability  to  stiffen  the  neck,  turn  the  head  at  will,  and  balance  it 
(3d  —  10th  weeks)  they  disappeared;  I  have  no  note  of  them  after 
the  first  month.  The  first  co-ordinated  leg-movements  I  saw  the 
baby  use,  —  propping  herself  with  her  knees  when  laid  on  her  face, 
—  were  on  the  32d  day;  and  during  the  next  week,  when  held  out 
in  one's  lap  along  the  knees,  she  began  to  push  strongly  with  her 
feet  against  the  body  of  the  person  who  held  her,  and  when  in  the 
bath,  against  the  foot  of  the  tub:  simultaneously,  the  aimless  leg- 
movements  disappeared,  and  are  noted  but  once  afterward.  Ann- 
movements  of  the  primitive  type  continued  longer,  through  the  sec- 
ond month,  increasing  in  freedom  ("waving,"  and  "brandishing"  arms 
are  the  words  I  find  in  my  notes):  but  within  a  week  after  the  first 
appearance  of  any  voluntary  arm-movement  (a  little  tugging  on  her 
mother's  hands  when  her  arms  were  held  up  in  wiping  her  after  the 
bath),  they  had  ceased  to  be  continuous:  she  would  lie  perfectly 
quiet  half  an  hour  at  a  time,  looking  about  her.  Vigorous  move- 
ments of  arms  and  legs  were  common  enough  after  this:  but  they 
were  of  a  different  type,  either  expressions  of  joy  and  excitement  or, 
in  my  judgment,  voluntarily  made  for  pleasure  in  the  muscular 
activity. 

'A  few  isolated  instances  of  movements  that  might   be  classed  with  this 
type  occurred  later,  and  are  noted  under  other  heads. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  1 8  I 

Such  motor  adjustments  as  showed  the  directing  power  of 
sensation  were  thus  present  in  the  eye-muscles  by  the  25th  day( 
and  in  quite  complete  degree  by  the  eighth  week;  in  the  neck 
muscles  by  the  third  week,  and  in  quite  complete  degree  by  the 
tenth.  There  must  have  been  an  advanced  degree  of  voluntary 
control  in  the  tongue  and  lips  by  the  seventh  week,  as  the  baby  at 
this  time  experimented  deliberately  with  them  (see  Feeling,  p.  136)- 
and  these  and  the  rest  of  the  vocal  organs  had  come  under  control 
sufficiently  for  experimenting  with  sounds  by  the  fourth  month. 
In  the  leg  muscles,  whether  the  guidance  of  sensation  was  neces- 
sary or  not  for  such  simple  acts  (possibly  reflex)  as  propping  with 
the  knees,  or  pushing  against  a  resisting  surface,  which  began  with 
the  second  month,  it  certainly  seemed  present  in  the  skilful  kicking 
that  appeared  in  the  latter  part  of  the  third  month.  The  trunk 
muscles  must  have  co-operated  in  the  propping  and  pushing  move- 
ments of  the  legs;  and  though  these  very  simple  contractions  may 
not  have  needed  the  guidance  of  sensation,  it  must  have  been 
involved  in  the  repeated  efforts  to  straighten  up  the  body  in  the 
first  week  of  the  third  month  (see  Movement).  In  the  arms,  its 
guidance  first  appeared  on  the  47th  day,  and  was  habitual  (in  efforts 
to  carry  the  hand  to  the  mouth)  by  the  first  week  of  the  third 
month.  In  neither  legs,  arms,  nor  trunk,  did  skill  of  movement 
reach  any  advanced  stage  within  the  first  year.  In  the  fingers,  it 
was  still  later:  the  first  attempts  at  grasping  extended  through  the 
fifth  month,  while  small  objects,  as  a  single  hair,  were  not  handled 
till  the  latter  part  of  the  eighth  month;  and  even  in  the  second  and 
third  years  finger  skill  was  very  limited.1 

1  The  order  here  is:  Eye,  neck,  tongue  and  lips,  trunk  and  arms,  legs,  vocal 
organs  in  general,  fingers.  It  is  evident,  however,  that  the  use  of  arms  and 
fingers  must  needs  wait  on  that  of  eyes,  since  grasping  is  quite  a  complex 
act,  in  which  sight,  touch,  and  muscle  sense  must  co-operate;  that  legs  and 
trunk  must  come  into  co-operation  before  any  but  the  simplest  action  is 
possible  for  either;  while  the  vocal  organs  have  co-operations  with  each 
other  to  acquire  before  much  advance  in  their  use  is  possible.  It  would  be 
hasty  to  say  that  after  these  preliminary  co-operations  are  once  acquired, 
the  order  of  progress  in  muscular  control  might  not  differ  considerably 
from  the  order  in  which  it  was  first  established.  As  a  matter  of  fact,  how- 
ever, I  am  quite  convinced,  from  later  observations,  that  the  eye  does 
keep  its  lead,  while  the  vocal  organs  come  rapidly  to  the  front  at  the  time  of 


1 82  University  of  California.  [Vol. i. 

The  most  abundant  evidence  of  the  existence  and  importance 
of  the  muscular  sensations,  (except  that  afforded  by  the  growth  of 
skill  in  action),  was  the  remarkable  pleasure  experienced  in  the 
exercise  of  the  muscles,  and  the  dislike  of  all  muscular  restraint. 

The  striking  satisfaction  experienced  in  liberation  from  clothes. 
I  put  down  as  due  to  relief  from  this  restraint;  though  there  proba- 
bly was  also  a  considerable  feeling  of  rest  in  the  cessation  of  con- 
tinuous touch  sensations  on  the  skin;  and  there  may  have  been  an 
increased  freedom  of  circulation,  though  the  baby's  garments  were 
carefully  planned  to  avoid  all  constriction.  From  the  first  month 
(my  note  fails  to  show  just  how  early)  the  expression  of  satisfaction 
in  the  baby's  face  when  she  was  being  undressed  for  the  bath,  or 
laid  naked  on  her  mother's  knees,  was  noticeable.  In  the  tenth 
week  I  note  that  undressing  and  the  bath  give  as  much  satisfaction 
as  food;  and  in  the  fifteenth  week  that  undressing  (with  the  bath 
or  rubbing  that  followed)  is  next  to  seeing  faces  and  being  played 
with  among  her  chief  joys,  —  calling  out  not  the  vivacious  expres- 
sions that  the  social  pleasures  did,  but  an  indescribable  look  of 
felicity,  with  opened  mouth  and  eager  gaze  at  her  mother's  face, 
and  only  occasional  vigorous  kicking.  By  the  sixth  month  it  made 
her  very  gay,  and  I  find  many  notes  such  as  "when  undressed, 
went  into  a  shower  of  gurgles,"  or  "laughed  abundantly."  In  some 
cases,  being  undressed  and  rubbed  changed  her  rnood,  leaving  her 
happy  and  sunny,  after  she  had  been  fretful  all  the  afternoon. 

During  the  period  in  the  third  and  fourth  months  when  she  was 
persistently  eager  to  sit  up,  fretting  inconsolably  when  laid  down 
under  ordinary  conditions,  she  was  still  content  and  often  hilarious 
if  laid  down  naked,  or  even  with  legs  free.  In  the  fourth  and  fifth 
months,  she  was  often  laid  in  the  sun  (her  head  shaded)  with  her 

acquiring  speech,  passing  the  trunk  and  limbs  in  development;  and  that  the 
fingers  remain  in  the  rear  for  years.  I  will  just  suggest  here  the  pedagogic 
significance  of  this  observation,  —  its  bearing,  for  instance,  on  the  question 
whether  reading  should  precede  hand  training,  so  far  as  the  question  is  one 
of  muscular  conditions,  —  hoping  to  recur  to  it  later,  with  fuller  material  for 
its  consideration.  As  to  the  bearing  on  the  formula  that  the  development  of 
muscular  control  proceeds  from  essential  to  subsidiary  muscles,  or,  as  some- 
times quoted,  from  central  to  peripheral  muscles,  it  is  to  be  said  that  discussions 
of  this  formula  have  not  attempted  to  take  into  account  the  head,  neck,  and  face 
muscles. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  I  S3 

legs  thus  free,  and  would  lie  in  high  spirits,  kicking,  crowing,  gur- 
gling, smiling,  and  chewing  the  rubber  of  her  rattle,  for  twenty- 
minutes  at  a  time:  a  few  instances  of  similar  jubilant  behavior  when 
she  was  laid  down  without  the  legs  free,  are  noted,  but  it  was  un- 
usual. In  the  ninth  month  she  was  several  times  allowed  to  creep 
about  on  very  hot  days  in  diaper  and  shirt,  and  it  gave  her  singular 
delight;  still  more  when  she  was  allowed  to  creep  naked.  In  the 
second  year  during  the  winter  months  (fifteenth  to  seventeenth)  she 
was  undressed  on  the  hearth-rug,  and  several  times  allowed  to 
caper  about  it  unclad:  it  gave  her  great  delight,  but  she  soon  tried 
to  escape  and  scamper  about  the  room.  This  she  was  allowed  to 
do  on  several  very  warm  days  of  the  following  summer  (eighteenth 
to  twenty-first  months)  after  she  was  undressed,  or  in  her  night- 
gown, —  which  came  to  much  the  same  thing,  as  she  held  it  up  high 
enough  to  free  her  legs  completely.  Her  joy  in  this  emancipation 
was  remarkable,  —  running  back  and  forth,  squealing,  shouting, 
gurgling,  flinging  herself  down  and  jumping  up,  in  an  ecstacy  of 
freedom.  Once  in  the  eighteenth  month  it  was  impossible  to  get 
her  to  leave  the  interesting  occupation  of  climbing  up  and  down 
stairs  for  the  sake  of  dinner,  and  she  had  to  be  carried  to  the  table 
forcibly;  yet  when  after  dinner  she  had  returned  with  zeal  to  the 
climbing,  she  instantly  deserted  it,  laughing  and  eager,  at  the  sug- 
gestion that  if  she  would  come  and  be  undressed  she  might  run 
around  in  her  nightgown.  Next  afternoon  she  began  to  ask 
for  the  same  indulgence,  and  cried  pitifully  with  disappointment 
(a  most  unusual  thing  with  her)  when  it  was  not  considered  warm 
enough;  and  she  did  not  give  it  up  as  usual,  but  renewed  the 
request  in  the  evening.  Although  she  was  as  a  rule  intensely  un- 
willing to  come  in  from  outdoors,  she  came  in  cheerfully  once,  in 
the  latter  part  of  the  twenty-second  month,  on  the  promise  that  she 
might  run  round  in  her  nightgown. 

With  the  third  year,  the  release  from  clothes  ceased  to  cause 
such  an  abandon  of  joy;  twice  in  the  twenty-fifth  month  I  note 
that  she  asked  prettily  when  undressed,  "May  Ruth  run  round  little 
bit?"  and  scampered  about,  trying  to  work  herself  up  to  the  expected 
hilarity,  and  laughing  not  altogether  spontaneously:  still,  she 
enjoyed  it  much,  and  nearly  cried  when  she  had  to  stop.  After 
this  she  either  ceased  to  seek  the  privilege,  or  the  colder  weather 


184  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Drought  it  to  an  end ;  I  have  no  farther  note  of  it.  She  continued 
more  or  less  disposed  to  "rampage"  in  her  bath,  however,  or  on 
her  mother's  bed  when  undressed,  instead  of  getting  into  her  own 
crib,  —  a  common  enough  observation  with  little  children,  —  as  if 
the  release  from  clothes  acted  as  a  stimulant  to  activity.1 

The  converse  feeling  of  dislike  to  being  dressed,  appears  a  little 
later  in  my  notes.  By  the  middle  of  the  second  month  complaints 
under  the  process  are  noted.  By  the  fourth  month  this  was  the 
main  source  of  displeasure  in  the  child's  existence;  she  almost 
always  whimpered  over  it,  and  if  she  was  tired  or  hungry  it  caused 
wails  and  tears.  Sleeves  were  a  special  grievance,  and  in  the  thirty- 
seventh  week  if  I  was  near  by  when  her  mother  came  to  this  part 
of  the  process,  the  baby  would  reach  her  arms  to  me  in  appeal  for 
rescue.  Up  to  the  time  that  she  had  learned  the  connection  of 
cloak  and  cap  with  going  outdoors  (late  in  the  seventh  month)  she 
complained  when  they  were  put  on.  In  her  baby-carriage  she  was 
usually  unwilling  to  keep  her  hands  under  cover.  By  the  last  week 
of  the  eleventh  month  her  dislike  of  dressing  had  declined,  and  as 
a  rule  she  would  lie  unconcernedly  across  one's  knee  and  let  herself 
be  dressed;  after  the  twelfth  month,  I  find  no  note  at  all  of  whim- 
pering over  it.  By  the  twenty-second  month,  however,  she  be- 
came very  averse  to  keeping  her  cap  on  outdoors,  wished  her 
head  free,  and  pulled  the  cap  off  constantly;  her  coat  too  she  dis- 
liked and  would  twitch  at,  saying,  "Ruth  don't  need  have  coat!'' 
(She  is  to  the  present  writing  —  at  seven  years  old  —  very  much 
disposed  to  get  hat  and  coat  off  outdoors,  though  she  has  no  dislike 
whatever  to  clothes  in  general.)  In  the  twenty-sixth  month,  when 
she  was  sleeping  in  my  charge  for  a  time,  I  used  to  muffle  her  up 
in  a  comforter  in  the  morning  while  I  went  to  prepare  her  bath,  as 
it  was  impossible  to  keep  her  lying  in  bed:  she  detested  this,  and 

'The  mere  novelty  of  playing  without  clothes  no  doubt  helped  to  stimulate 
excitement  (as  the  loss  in  excitement  in  the  twenty-fifth  month  goes  to  show): 
but  scarcely  in  the  first  year  ;  nor  even  in  the  second  and  third  was  it  enough 
to  account  for  all  the  exhilaration  shown.  Even  adults  are  aware  sometimes 
of  a  quite  remarkable  sense  of  relief  and  heightening  of  energy  in  disrobing  or 
putting  on  freer  and  lighter  clothes.  That  there  is  real  muscular  restraint  in 
clothes  is  evident  from  the  necessity  athletes  find  of  stripping  as  much  as  possi- 
ble before  exercise. 


Shinn.]  ,      The  Development  of  a   Child.  1 85 

it  took  some  firmness  to  prevent  her  throwing  off  the  wrappings, 
however  cold  it  might  be;  on  one  occasion  I  came  back  and  found 
her  sitting  obediently,  still  swathed  to  the  chin,  but  in  tears. 

One  of  the  earliest  signs  of  anger  I  ever  detected  in  the  baby,  — 
a  tone  of  temper  in  crying,  —  was  on  occasion  of  an  extra  dressing 
one  day,  at  eleven  months  old;  and  thereafter  whenever  she  cried 
over  being  dressed  there  was  the  same  slight  tone  of  temper.  There 
quickly  appeared  now  a  resentment  against  muscular  restraint  of 
any  sort:  to  be  held  still  in  order  to  have  her  clothes  arranged,  to 
be  kept  fastened  in  her  high  chair  or  nursery  chair  when  she  wished 
to  get  away,  caused  angry  crying  and  efforts  to  escape,  —  almost 
the  only  signs  of  temper  she  ever  showed  at  this  period.  In  the 
thirteenth  month,  if  she  was  taken  or  held  against  her  wish,  she 
would  either  stiffen  and  wriggle  away,  or  lift  her  arms  and  become 
limp,  so  that  she  slipped  through  one's  grasp.  I  note  squealing, 
stiffening,  and  wriggling,  in  protest  against  being  taken  or  held,  as 
late  as  the  nineteenth  month,  and  it  is  my  impression  that  it  lasted 
considerably  longer;  in  the  eighteenth  month,  she  would  twist  her- 
self away  from  an  annoying  hold.  She  never  cared  much  to  be 
held  in  arms;  rarely  would  consent  to  sit  long  in  anyone's  lap;  and 
after  getting  the  use  of  her  own  legs,  almost  never  asked  to  be 
taken  or  carried.  After  she  was  a  year  and  a  half  old  I  find  con- 
stant notes  of  her  reluctance  to  be  carried,  held,  or  even  led.  In 
the  twentieth  month  she  would  cry,  "Ruth  walk!"  or  "Own  self!" 
if  lifted  in  arms,  and  pull  away  her  hand,  crying,  "No !"  if  one  tried 
to  lead  her;  she  pushed  away  my  hand  when  I  held  her  dress  as 
she  climbed  a  risky  place  (or,  the  next  month,  as  she  swung  in  the 
hammock).  In  the  twenty-first  month,  as  at  a  year  old,  though 
she  never  showed  real  passion,  restraint  could  make  her  cross  for 
a  few  minutes,  and  restraint  only:  she  would  squirm  and  scold  to 
get  away,  to  be  allowed  to  get  down  from  her  chair,  to  go  out  of  a 
room.  "Let  Ruth  go !",  "Let  Ruth  down  !",  "Don't !  "  she  would 
scold.  "Staking  out,"  that  she  might  play  about  safely  without 
much  watching,  an  admirable  device  with  some  babies,  proved 
impracticable  with  her:  except  in  one  instance,  when  she  was  so 
occupied  that  she  perhaps  did  not  discover  she  was  tied,  she  would 
immediately  find  the  end  of  her  tether  and  be  driven  to  despair. 
In  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  was  very  fond  of  "playing  catch," 


1 86  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

and  would  coax  her  uncle  into  it,  making  sallies  and  calling  him  stren- 
uously :  she  did  not  mind  being  caught  occasionally,  but  objected 
greatly  to  being  held,  and  if  he  did  not  release  her  at  once  would 
first  cry,  "Let  me  go  ! "  then  appeal,  "Aunty,  come  help  me  !  Uncle 
Joe  did  catch  me  ! "  with  a  most  aggrieved  air.  In  the  twenty-eighth 
month,  when  her  grandmother  once  held  her  tight  to  prevent  her 
running  into  some  danger,  she  became  much  displeased,  told  her  to 
"go  away,"  and  refused  to  kiss  her  for  many  minutes,  which  was 
unusual  resentment  for  her  sunny  temperament. 

After  this,  she  became  more  willing  to  be  carried,  or  to  sit  on 
our  laps,  but  I  still  find  notes  of  appeals  to  be  put  down,  and  as 
late  as  the  thirty-fifth  month,  she  reported  to  me  that  she  had 
"s'apped  damma,"  angry  at  being  taken  from  the  bath  and  held  still 
to  check  her  capering  till  she  could  be  wiped. 

Even  the  mere  suggestion  of  restraint,  a  caress  or  touch,  some- 
times annoyed  her  keenly  in  the  second  year.  In  the  eighteenth 
month,  her  mother's  hands  laid  on  the  back  of  her  chair  annoyed 
her;  she  put  up  her  own  hand,  and  pushed  first  one  and  then  the 
other  away,  saying,  "'Way!"  and  in  the  twentieth,  pushed  mine  off 
with  the  uncivil  remark,  "Ta'  bi'  ha'  'way"  —  Take  big  hands  away. 
So  again  in  the  twenty-third  month.  In  the  twentieth  month  she 
seemed  annoyed  at  caresses,  did  not  want  a  face  or  hand  too  near  her, 
and  would  push  it  away  saying,  "Take  head  away!"  "Take  hand 
away  ! "  The  day  she  was  twenty  months  old  her  father  came  in  as  she 
was  undressed.and  took  her  foot  in  his  hand.  "Papa.don't  touch  Ruth 
foot!"  she  cried:  "don't  touch  Ruth  foot!  don't  touch  baby  foot!" 
"Don'tkiss  hand!"  "Don't  eat  [kiss]  Ruth  neck!"  "Don't  kiss 
Ruth,  —  hurts! "  are  noted  on  one  day  in  the  twenty-first  month;  a  few 
days  later  I  kissed  her  hand  when  she  was  already  squirming  to  get 
away  from  her  father's  hold,  and  she  fairly  snarled,  "Don't!  "  snatch- 
ing her  hand  away.  In  the  twenty-second  month,  she  tumbled  about 
happily  half  an  hour  on  my  bed,  listening  to  Mother  Goose  rhymes 
and  wishing  me  to  lie  near  her,  but  utterly  refused  to  let  me  put  an 
arm  about  her  or  touch  her,  saying  crossly,  "Don't ! "  —  "Don't 
what?"  —  "Put  arm  on  Ruth  back,"  —  i.  e.,  around  her.  She  would 
sometimes  give  caresses  herself,  but  wished  to  be  left  free  to  retreat 
on  the  instant:  so  in  the  twenty-fourth  month,  lying  beside  me,  she 
would  roll  up  and  kiss  me  gayly,  then  away  again.     It  was  some- 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  1 87 

thing  like  a  half-tamed  kitten  that  will  offer  caresses,  but  springs 
away  with  fear  of  being  caught  if  you  lay  your  hand  on  it:  but  in 
the  child's  case  there  was  no  sign  of  fear,  only  repugnance.  With 
the  close  of  the  second  year  this  feeling  about  caresses  ceased  to  be 
noticeable.  In  the  last  week  of  the  year,  having  coaxed  me  to  lie 
down  on  the  floor  by  her,  she  wished  me  close  to  her,  and  stowed 
herself  in  my  arms,  across  my  breast  or  head,  face  against  mine, 
repeatedly  kissing;  and  at  intervals  during  the  third  year,  when 
sleeping  beside  me,  she  liked  to  creep  over  into  my  bed  and  go  to 
sleep  in  my  arms ;  "Lie  c'ose  to  me,  au'tee,"  she  would  say.  She 
was,  however,  considerably  less  clinging  by  daylight  than  in  the 
dark. 

Late  in  the  twenty-eighth  month  she  made  great  resistance  to 
having  a  poultice  put  on  her  finger,  running  away  and  crying,  "No, 
mamma,  no!"  After  some  reasoning,  she  submitted  to  the  sub- 
stitute of  a  glove-finger  and  salve,  and  kept  it  on  all  day.  Such 
whims  were  not  frequent  with  her ;  and  the  only  interpretation  I 
could  make  of  her  vague  but  strong  dread,  was  that  it  was  con- 
nected with  her  repugnance  to  being  in  any  way  bound. 

From  about  the  middle  of  the  seventeenth  month,  "Caught! 
caught ! "  was  her  cry,  to  express  the  distressing  feeling  of  being 
impeded,  or  held,  as  when  wedged  in  a  narrow  space  between  wall 
and  chair,  or  when  her  dress  caught  and  held  her  back..  Fastened 
in  her  high  chair  (the  first  day  of  the  eighteenth  month)  she  strug- 
gled to  get  out,  crying,  "Cau'!  cau'!"  She  would  cry,"Cau'!"  if  held 
in  our  arms  when  she  wished  to  go  free ;  if  two  chairs  were  too 
near  together  to  allow  her  to  pass  between  them  easily,  she  would 
complain,  "Cau' !",  as  soon  as  she  found  she  would  have  to  squeeze 
to  get  through ;  or  if  any  twig  or  vine  tangled  about  her  feet,  the 
same  cry  came,  —  sometimes  when  the  impediment  was  absurdly 
small,  a  mere  suggestion  of  hindrance,  as  when  a  forked  seed-vessel 
of  Martynia  hooked  itself  about  her  leg.  In  the  nineteenth  month 
she  cried  when  side-garters  were  first  put  on,  pulling  at  them  and 
saying,  "Cau' !  cau'!"  and  "Off!"  when  she  felt  the  slight  tension,  as 
she  moved.  Once  in  the  twenty-first  month  she  climbed  up  on 
the  lower  rail  of  a  fence,  and  after  trying  in  vain  to  get  up  to  the 
next  one,  complained,  "Ruth  caught,  —  little  foot  caught!"  though 
nothing  but  gravitation    impeded  the  little  foot. 


1 88  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

The  first  sign  of  imaginative  sympathy  she  showed  was  for  this 
grievance  of  restraint,  when  she  tried  to  lift  away  the  branch  from 
a  picture  of  a  lamb  caught  in  briers  (twenty-fifth  month,  see  Sight, 
p.  104).  In  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  nearly  cried  over  a  pic- 
ture of  an  opossum  holding  a  bird  in  his  mouth,  —  not  because  the 
bird  might  be  hurt  or  killed,  but  because  it  was  held:  "Mamma, 
that  little  bird  can't  get  away!"  she  said  pitifully.  In  the  twenty- 
eighth  month  she  saw  a  photograph  of  the  well-known  Faun  and 
Child,  and  asked  in  a  troubled  tone  if  the  child  could  not  get  down; 
and  again,  comingtosome  pictures  of  papooses  in  their  cases,  "Can't 
the  baby  get  out?"  Of  a  child  in  abed  whose  covers  were  fastened 
down,  "That  baby  caught !  He  can't  get  out!  That  baby  tied!" 
Up  to  this  date,  her  pity  had  never  (with  a  single  exception)  been 
excited  by  picture  or  story  for  any  grievance  save  this  one  of  phys- 
ical restraint. 

There  was  no  doubt  more  in  this  curiously  strong  dread  of  be- 
ing caught,  held,  impeded,  than  mere  discomfort  in  the  restraint  of 
muscular  activity.  Some  of  it  might  be  attributed  to  a  more  gen- 
eral desire  of  freedom,  though  this  particular  child  did  not  other- 
wise show  much  passion  for  the  free  exercise  of  her  will,  yielding  it 
rather  easily  as  a  rule.  There  was  often  something  curiously  un- 
reasoning and  instinctive  in  manner  in  the  repugnance  she  showed 
to  physical  restraint,  which  suggested  remote  inheritance,  savage 
or  even  pre-human.  In  other  manifestations,  as  the  dislike  to 
hands  on  her  chair,  or  the  exclamation,  "That  Rutli  foot!"  there 
seemed  a  glimpse  of  a  sensitive  defense  of  personality  (something 
quite  different  from  mere  assertion  of  will)  that  was  strangely  ma- 
ture, and  in  advance  of  her  general  stage  of  development. 

Enjoyment  of  active  muscular  exercise  for  its  own  sake  did  not 
appear  very  early.  I  saw  no  instance  of  it  till  late  in  the  third 
month,1  when  the  baby,  lying  naked  in  her  mother's  lap,  would 
kick  out  her  legs  with  a  new  motion,  quite  unlike  the  former  vague 
motions,  straight  and  strong,  with  as  much  precision  as  if  she  were 

1  But  see  under  Movements  (below)  one  or  two  instances  of  ,1  curious  jerk- 
ing and  turning  of  the  head  in  the  last  week  of  the  second  month;  and  also  note 
the  pushing  with  the  feet  in  the  second  and  third  months. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  189 

performing  a  gymnastic  exercise,  and  with  every  appearance  ot  enjoy- 
ment. This  hilarious  kicking  was  very  common  thereafter  for  sev- 
eral months,  till  creeping  was  acquired,  in  the  ninth  month.  It  was 
always  quite  different  from  the  demonstrations  with  legs  and  arms 
that  expressed  joy  and  excitement:  I  was  satisfied  that  it  was  done 
purely  for  the  pleasure  of  the  sensation.  In  the  third  month  also, 
86th  day,  I  noticed  that  when  holding  her  rattle  and  flourishing  her 
arms  she  no  longer  flourished  the  other  arm,  and  I  thought  the 
movement  was  now  deliberate,  for  pleasure  in  brandishing  the  rattle. 

In  the  fourth  month  began  the  making  of  various  sounds  for 
amusement,  in  which  the  pleasure  must  have  been  as  much  in  the 
muscular  sensation  as  the  noises ;  this  continued  through  the  next 
month,  and  was  recurred  to  at  intervals  during  the  whole  period 
under  consideration.  In  the  fourteenth  week,  it  amused  the  baby 
much  to  sneeze,  and  she  always  ended  with  a  bright  smile. 
"Playing"  with  objects  began  in  the  fourth  month,  also,  —  at 
first  merely  fumbling  with  rattles  and  other  easily  handled  things, 
moving  them  about  and  putting  them  to  the  mouth :  then  more 
complex  hand  occupations  (such  as  tearing  paper  to  bits  in 
the  sixth  month,  thumping  the  piano  in  the  eighth,  rocking  the 
sewing  machine  pedal  or  pulling  things  out  of  a  basket  in  the  ninth  ; 
rocking  chairs,  raising  and  shutting  lids,  pulling  doors  to  and  fro, 
digging  in  the  dirt,  waving  good-by,  shaking  hands,  in  the  last 
quarter  of  the  year;  and  a  multitude  of  more  and  more  complicated 
acts  in  the  second  and  third  yearsj.  In  all  these  there  was  a  certain 
element  of  muscular  pleasure,  though  obscure. 

From  the  first  day  of  the  fifth  month  I  note  splashing  in  the 
bath,  also  a  complex  pleasure,  with  an  element  of  muscular  sensa- 
tion. In  the  same  month,  I  began  to  see  instances  of  especial  pleas- 
ure in  exercising  muscle  against  resistance.1  The  baby  used  to  take 
firmly  between  her  jaws  a  stout  rubber  strip  attached  to  her  rattle, 
and  twitch  it  out  with  a  jerk,  getting  of  course  a  double  sensation, 

1  There  may  have  been  some  such  liking  in  the  pushing  with  her  feet  hori- 
zontally in  the  second  month  (p.  180),  and  in  the  downward  pushing,  "feeling 
her  feet,"  that  preceded  walking  movements;  but  it  is  more  likely  that  these 
were  involuntary  responses  to  pressure  on  the  soles,  connected  with  the  unde- 
veloped instinct  to  stand  and  walk.  Mrs.  Beatty's  boy  (seep.  135,  above),  on 
the  16th  day,  pushed  so  strongly  against  the  foot  of  his  basket  that  he  nearly 
pushed  himself  out  of  it. 


1  9°  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

in  arms  and  in  jaws.  Perhaps  a  similar  muscular  pleasure  was 
originally  the  motive  of  a  persistent  habit,  dating  from  the  first  week 
of  this  month,  —  that  of  tugging  at  her  father's  and  grandfather's 
whiskers,  which  they  somewhat  fatuously  encouraged.  At  the  end 
of  the  sixth  month,  grasping  being  now  fully  established,  she 
became  most  eager  to  pull,  with  laughter  and  exultant  clamor,  at  the 
nose,  ear,  and  especially  the  hair,  of  any  one  that  held  her.  In  the 
seventh  and  eighth  months,  she  not  only  assailed  her  mother  and 
me  thus,  but  had  romps  of  the  sort  with  several  young  girls  of  the 
neighborhood,  who  liked  to  get  down  on  the  hearth  rug  beside  her 
and  surrender  their  bangs  to  the  rollicking  little  thing.  We  had 
soon  to  check  the  habit,  but  in  the  latter  part  of  the  tenth  month, 
and  through  the  eleventh,  I  note  seizing  and  pulling  hair  as  a  trait 
of  rough  frolics.  From  the  seventh  month  on,  there  was  from  time 
to  time  shown  a  curiously  intense  desire  to  get  hold  of  and  tug  at 
the  hair  of  the  dog,  the  cat's  fur,  even  the  fur  of  a  rug;  pulling  the 
dog's  ears  and  tail  was  inveterate  at  times.  How  far  all  this  was  a 
case  of  a  general  liking  to  use  muscular  effort  against  a  resistance, 
I  could  form  no  judgment.  From  the  time  grasping  was  acquired, 
there  often  seemed  a  special  satisfaction  in  pulling  at  a  fastened 
object,  more  than  in  obtaining  possession  of  a  free  one.  In  the 
twelfth  month  the  baby  was  given  a  bit  of  tough  steak  to  suck,  but 
found  more  pleasure  in  tearing  it  with  her  jaws,  much  as  she  had 
pulled  at  the  strip  of  rubber  in  the  fifth  month. 

With  the  latter  half  of  the  first  year  came  a  period  of  peculiar 
pleasure  in  the  movements  of  her  body,  connected  with  the  acquisi- 
tion of  the  main  race-movements,  —  rolling,  creeping,  standing, 
walking.  In  some  cases,  the  acquisition  of  these  movements  was 
sought  very  seriously,  merely  as  a  means  to  an  end,  and  in  others 
appeared  accidental  or  almost  automatic;  but  in  still  others,  it  was 
attended  with  great  joy  in  the  exercise  (see  Movements,  below). 
On  the  whole,  as  her  power  over  her  body  increased,  her  chief  joy 
came  to  be  in  the  free  use  of  her  muscles.  Thus  it  is  my  constant 
note  during  the  seventh  and  eighth  months  that  she  lay  kicking 
and  rolling  about  the  floor,  or  on  a  blanket  spread  on  veranda  or 
lawn,  perfectly  happy  by  the  hour,  sometimes  breaking  into. cries 
and  movements  of  joy.  In  the  eighth  month  if  one  carried  her  about 
in  the  arms,  she  refused  to  be  entertained  by  any  efforts,  and  leaned 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  191 

over  and  looked  wistfully  at  the  ground  or  floor,  to  indicate  her 
wish  to  be  put  down  and  left  to  the  freedom  of  her  own  movements. 
The  kicking  (p.  188)  at  this  time  was  of  an  extraordinary  vigor:  she 
would  raise  both  feet  as  high  as  she  could  and  bring  them  down 
with  the  utmost  force:  it  seemed  unaccountable  that  it  did  not  bruise 
her  heels  badly,  but  she  appeared  to  take  the  greatest  delight  in  the 
exercise.  For  several  days  in  the  latter  part  of  the  eighth  month, 
instead  of  this  kicking,  she  would  sometimes  hold  her  legs  up  per- 
pendicularly for  several  seconds.1  By  the  middle  of  the  ninth  month, 
the  rolling  and  kicking  were  succeeded  by  a  sort  of  varied  scram- 
bling about  the  floor,  — sitting  up  and  lying  down,  kneeling,  creep- 
ing, etc.;  then  in  the  next  three  months  standing  up  and  sitting 
down  and  some  beginnings  of  walking  were  added.  During  these 
months  I  constantly  note  her  delight  in  being  turned  loose  on  the 
floor,  or  better  yet  the  lawn,  where  she  would  tumble  about  at  will 
with  frequent  shouts,  crows,  and  babble  of  joy. 

Sundry  minor  exhibitions  of  pleasure  in  the  use  of  her  muscles 
were  noted  in  this  second  half-year.  In  the  twenty-ninth  week,  she 
practiced  grimacing  and  sniffing  for  some  days,  in  evident  gayety; 
in  the  thirty-first,  when  her  mother  put  her  hand  under  her  head  to 
support  it  in  the  bath,  she  would  begin  to  stiffen  and  throw  her 
body,  resting  on  her  feet  and  her  mother's  hand.  In  the  thirty- 
third  week,  when  she  was  taken  under  the  arms  and  swung  out,  she 
would  lift  up  her  body  and  legs  and  straighten  her  back  by  sheer 
muscle  (getting  help  of  course  from  the  motion  with  which  she  was 
swung)  till  she  was  horizontal,  or  even  till  her  feet  were  higher  than 
her  head,  and  would  keep  the  position  a  second  or  two.  In  the 
same  week  she  lifted  with  her  left  hand  a  valise  that  weighed  four 
pounds,  and  held  it  about  ten  seconds;  then  several  times  immedi- 
ately afterward  lifted  it  again  and  held  it  for  four  or  five  seconds. 
The  feeling  of  weight  on  her  arms  seemed  agreeable.  In  the 
eleventh  month  she  took  the  greatest  delight  in  the  newly  acquired 
accomplishment  of  nodding,  and  for  several  days  bobbed  her  head 
and  shoulders  diligently. 

1  In  the  same  month,  Preyer's  child  had  the  habit  of  holding  up  his  legs 
vertically,  but  with  somewhat  different  behavior;  he  was  interested  in  the  sight 
of  his  feet,  and  grasped  at  them,  while  my  niece  seemed  interested  only  in  the 
muscular  exercise,  and  learned  to  grasp  at  her  feet  and  carry  her  toes  to  her 
mouth  under  other  conditions. 


192  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

In  one  way  and  another  throughout  the  first  year  her  movement 
was  almost  incessant,  and  any  occasional  period  of  sitting  quietly 
(as  once  in  the  twenty-sixth  week  for  an  hour  in  church)  is  noted 
as  unusual. 

With  the  second  year  came  a  period  in  which,  the  main  race- 
movements  being  now  acquired,  occupations  of  a  more  mental  sort 
came  to  the  front,  and  pictures,  language,  sights,  exploring  and 
satisfying  curiosity,  overshadowed  the  simple  muscular  activities. 
Many  of  these  more  complex  occupations,  however,  involved  a  great 
deal  of  muscular  action,  so  that  the  child  was  still  perpetually  in 
movement;  and  in  some  it  was  hard  to  find  much  motive  beyond 
that  of  using  her  muscles.  The  liking  for  a  feeling  of  weight  men- 
tioned above  was  an  example.  In  the  thirteenth  month,  when  first 
walking,  she  liked  to  carry  about  a  weight,  doll  or  book,  merely  for 
the  sake  of  carrying  it,  and  the  fancy  recurred  at  intervals  through- 
out the  year.  At  the  beginning  of  the  seventeenth  month  she  lifted 
a  rubber  bag  of  water  that  weighed  seven  pounds;  in  the  eighteenth 
month  she  took  up  a  seven-pound  flat-iron  in  one  hand  and  walked 
off  with  it;  in  the  twentieth,  she  picked  up  a  two-pound  dumb-bell  in 
each  hand,  held  them  a  few  seconds,  and  then  dropped  them.  In  the 
nineteenth  month,  finding  a  pile  of  pamphlets  in  a  waste-basket,  she 
carried  twelve,  one  at  a  time,  into  the  next  room  to  me;  and  in  the 
twenty-second  month  she  carried  fifteen  sticks  of  wood,  in  like 
manner,  from  one  pile  to  another.  Here,  too,  may  be  mentioned 
the  pleasure  the  child  experienced  in  suspending  her  own  weight 
by  her  hands.  In  the  seventeenth  month  I  tried  lifting  her  by  a 
stick  held  in  her  hands.1  When  she  understood  the  idea  and  was 
supplied  a  stick  that  fitted  her  hands,  she  enjoyed  it  greatly,  laughed 
aloud  all  the  time  she  hung  from  the  stick,  and  was  eager  to  do  it 
over  and  over.  Each  time,  her  hands  relaxed  suddenly,  and  she 
fell,  with  an  expression  of  dismay;  but  was  very  eager  to  renew  the 
exercise.  In  a  few  days  she  took  an  aversion  to  it  and  refused  to 
repeat  it,  but  with  no  appearance  of  fear.  In  the  twenty-third 
month  she  returned  to  it  and  took  pleasure  in  hanging  for  a  few 

1  Darwinism    in  the    Nursery.      Dr.    Louis    Robinson,    Popular    Science 
Monthly,  vol.  40,  p.  674. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  193 

seconds;  and  again  in  the  twenty-seventh  month.  (From  her  fourth 
year  on,  the  child  has  been  supplied  with  a  simple  apparatus  —  a 
rope  and  stick  —  to  swing  from,  and  has  had  much  pleasure  in  it; 
and  by  the  sixth  year  she  was  bold  and  skilful  in  swinging  from  the 
branches  of  trees.)  In  the  twenty-first  month,  finding  two  chairs  in 
the  middle  of  the  veranda,  she  pushed  one  to  the  extreme  west  end 
of  the  veranda,  turning  the  corner  of  the  house,  then  turned  the 
chair  about  and  pushed  it  back  around  two  corners  to  the  extreme 
east  end;  then  remarking,  "  Get  another  chair,"  brought  the  other 
likewise,  —  a  distance  in  all  of  about  250  feet,  over  a  stone-paved 
floor. 

By  the  latter  half  of  the  second  year  the  higher  development  and 
variety  of  the  bodily  movements  now  attained,  had  once  more  made 
them  a  source  of  very  considerable  pleasure  in  themselves,  apart 
from  the  mental  interests  they  opened  up;  as  will  be  more  fully 
reported  under  the  head  of  Movements.  By  the  end  of  the  year, 
however,  the  free  running  and  walking,  jumping,  climbing,  etc.,  had 
ceased  to  be  so  much  enjoyed  for  their  own  sakes,  and  mental 
interests  again  distinctly  overshadowed  muscular  pleasure. 

Ebullitions  of  the  liveliest  frisking,  racing,  jumping,  shouting, 
with  intense  desire  for  motion  and  noise,  are  noted  from  time  to 
time  (especially  in  the  twenty-first  month);  it  seemed  impossible,  up 
to  the  third  year,  for  the  child  to  sit  without  an  occupation,  simply 
looking  about.  All  this  exuberant  action  was  doubtless  in  part  a 
mere  spontaneous  discharge  of  energy,  the  expression  of  a  high 
general  well-being,  and  in  part  an  indulgence  in  muscular  action  for 
the  pleasure  of  the  sensation  it  afforded. 

Though  she  had  been  from  very  early  a  "romping  baby,"  fond 
of  rough  play,  the  romping  of  the  first  year  had  necessarily  been 
of  a  sort  in  which  she  was  rather  passive  (as  being  tossed  or 
danced);  but  by  the  fourteenth  month,  she  began  to  find  the  joy  of 
active  romping.  In  the  fifty-eighth  week,  she  frolicked  with  the 
dog,  running  at  him  with  laughter.  The  sixty-fifth  week  she  dis- 
covered the  charm  of  "playing  catch"  around  tables  and  chairs, 
with  hilarious  glee.  No  doubt  the  pleasure  was  partly  in  the 
dramatic  element  of  the  play  (two  days  later,  e.g.,  she  laughed  her- 
self weak  when  quietly  playing  hide  and  seek  behind  the  furniture, 


194  University  of  California.  [vol.  i 

and  could  not  tire  of  it);  but  that  an  intense  muscular  pleasure 
blends  with  the  dramatic  interest  in  the  active  games  even  of  adults, 
any  one  knows  that  has  taken  part  in  athletic  contests.1  In  the 
seventeenth  month  (seventy-first  week)  she  was  delighted  with  a  boy 
some  seven  years  older  who  played  catch  with  her;  as  he  put  it,  "I 
chased  her  round  the  table,  and  now  she  wants  me  all  the  time." 
When  he  left  the  room,  she  would  follow,  crying  "Way!  Way!" 
(Ray,  his  name)  and  seeking  till  she  found  him.  In  the  seventy- 
fourth  week  she  was  very  happy  again  in  romping  with  him,  though 
he  was  rough  with  her  and  often  knocked  her  over,  at  which  she 
never  cried.  She  especially  desired  to  be  chased  by  him,  laughing 
all  the  time  as  she  ran.  Through  the  nineteenth  month  she  con- 
stantly wished  to  play  "catch,"  running  and  laughing  till  she  could 
hardly  stand.  In  the  last  week  of  the  month  she  was  in  wild 
spirits,  romping  with  the  same  nine-year-old  boy;  and  when  he 
rolled  her  over,  threw  a  blanket  over  her  head,  and  otherwise  used 
her  roughly,  she  took  it  in  the  highest  glee.  She  took  riotous 
pleasure  in  romping  with  the  dog  during  the  latter  months  of  the 
year.  On  the  last  day  of  the  twentieth  month,  as  she  was  frolick- 
ing on  the  lawn  in  the  highest  spirits,  he  knocked  her  down  by  a 
sudden  excited  rush.  She  laughed  heartily,  called  him  back,  and 
as  he  capered  about  near  her  she  went  into  ecstasies  of  laughter, 
but  took  care  to  sit  down  each  time  he  approached,  to  save  herself 
a  fall.  Once  when  he  rushed  very  close  by  her,  almost  grazing  her 
as  she  sat,  she  nearly  cried  with  laughter.  She  would  climb  all 
over  him,  clutching  him  roughly,  pulling  his  tail  and  hair,  shouting 
and  squealing,  and  crying,  "Sit  on  Muzhik!"  She  was  especially 
pleased  if  he  got  up  and  walked  off,  tipping  her  over,  and  would 
call,  "Get  off!"  and  "Get  up!"  to  intimate  her  desire  to  have  him 
do  this.  As  the  big  Russian  dog,  though  reasonably  good-natured, 
did  not  seem  over-fond  of  her  handling,  we  doubted  the  safety  of 
such  play;  but  she  was  bewitched  with  him,  and  when  he  was 
driven  from  her,  set  out  to  find  him  and  flung  herself  on  him  with 
joy.     After  she  had  finally  teased  him  into  snapping  at  her  (twenty- 

1  It  seems  to  be  characteristic  of  all  unspecialized  sensation  that  while  its 
effect  on  the  feelings  is  remarkably  strong,  and  the  part  it  plays  in  life  enor- 
mous, it  is  to  be  found  only  as  a  substratum  in  activities  of  which  something 
far  more  complex  and  definite  forms  the  obvious  part. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  195 

fourth  month)  and  was  greatly  interested  in  the  experience  and 
determined  to  repeat  it  (see  Feeling,  p.  1 50,  above),  the  dog  was 
given  away.  She  was  equally  fearless  with  another  dog  that  was 
occasionally  about,  climbed  all  over  him,  and  did  not  mind  rough 
play. 

Her  romping  took  an  unpleasant  turn,  quite  different  from  per- 
missible frolics,  in  an  occasional  impulse  to  strike  with  a  stick,  or 
(in  the  latter  months  of  the  second  year)  to  fly  rudely  at  some  one 
who  had  her  in  arms,  scratching  or  striking  at  the  face,  slapping 
and  clawing  at  the  hair,  even  biting  a  little,  always  in  good  temper, 
apparently  as  a  rough  joke.  I  note  this  from  the  seventeenth 
month  till  the  end  of  the  twenty-first.  In  the  last  quarter  of  the  sec- 
ond year  it  was  rare;  but  once  at  the  beginning  of  the  twenty-second 
month  (seventy-second  week-)  when  lifted  to  kiss  good-night,  she 
snatched  off  her  grandmother's  glasses,  then  her  cap,  and  flung 
them  to  the  floor,  likewise  her  grandfather's  glasses  when  the 
chance  came,  and  refused  to  kiss  any  of  us  good-night,  laughing  and 
romping  instead.  In  the  last  week  of  the  second  year  she  would 
stop  to  ask  with  amusing  politeness,  "May  I  pull  your  hair,  aunty  ?  " 
"May  I  bang  you,  aunty?"  and  would  give  up  the  intention  on 
receiving  an  emphatic  no. 

With  the  third  year,  as  implied  above,  the  great  interest  in  mus- 
cular exertion  that  had  accompanied  both  the  earlier  and  later 
development  of  the  race-movements,  disappeared.  In  the  acquire- 
ment of  these  movements  in  the  second  half-year,  and  afterward  in 
the  period  of  delight  in  the  fuller  development  (fourth  half-year), 
the  child  had  been  energetic  in  efforts  that  taxed  her  strength  and 
skill  to  the  utmost;  but  now  this  was  only  partly  so.  In  the 
twenty-seventh  month,  I  note  that  she  is  eager  to  try  novel  and 
difficult  things;  but  on  the  whole  there  was  a  disinclination  to  occu- 
pations that  taxed  her  muscular  skill;  in  the  thirtieth  month,  I  note 
avoidance  even  of  those  that  called  merely  for  muscular  exertion 
without  much  demand  of  skill.  "Too  hard,  — -you  do  it,"  she 
would  say.  This  was  in  striking  contrast  with  her  zeal  in  the  use 
of  her  eyes,  ears,  and  mind.  There  were  many  exceptions,  how- 
ever, to  her  dislike  to  hard  muscular  effort  in  this  half-year ;  at  times 
she  would  pull  and  tug  to  climb,  and  dig  till  she  was  red  in  the  face. 
14 


1 96  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

She  was,  in  a  more  desultory  way,  about  as  active  in  this  year  as  in 
the  second:  the  ebullitions  of  frisking,  racing,  and  jumping  con- 
tinued, and  notes  of  her  sitting  quietly  without  visible  occupation, 
prattling  to  herself  or  seeming  in  thought,  though  they  do  occur, 
are  very  few.  We  rarely  ventured  to  take  her  to  church,  but  tried 
it  once,  at  Easter  (when  she  was  two  years  and  a  half  old);  she 
proved  hopelessly  restless,  and  soon  got  up  and  walked  outdoors. 
Brought  back  toward  the  end  of  the  service,  she  behaved  very  ill, 
scrambled  about  the  seat  and  kicked  up  her  heels,  in  marked  con- 
trast to  the  decorous  behavior  of  several  other  little  things  of  her 
age.  (Even  at  seven  years  old,  she  cannot  sit  the  sermon  quietly 
through,  without  occupation,  as  they  do,  but  draws  and  scribbles 
on  a  paper,  reads  the  hymns,  etc.) 

She  still  experienced  great  joy  in  a  good  romp,  but  not  as  much 
in  proportion  to  other  pleasures  as  in  the  second  year.  Once  in 
her  twenty-fifth  month  a  boy  of  six  so  won  her  heart  by  romping 
and  running  with  her  that  whenever  he  stooped  for  any  reason  she 
would  seize  the  chance  to  run  up  and  embrace  him.  Her  games  of 
chasing  took  the  more  dramatic  form  of  "playing  bear,"  with  much 
springing,  growling,  running,  and  seizing,  peals  of  laughter  and 
shouts  of  delight.  Other  active  frolics  were  also  sought:  e.g.,  if 
I  lay  on  the  lounge,  she  loved  to  fling  herself  upon  me,  tumbling 
and  trampling  over  me,  and  to  be  pushed  and  rolled  off,  taking  and 
giving  as  rough  play  as  was  possible  without  harming  her,  with 
the  greatest  merriment  and  the  utmost  recklessness  on  her  part. 
(A  hard  romp,  especially  with  father  or  uncle,  is  enjoyed  still, 
at  seven  years;  most  of  all  a  pillow-fight,  in  which  she  asks  little 
consideration  for  her  size  and  strength,  and  takes  pretty  heavy  blows 
with  merriment.) 

She  had  been  broken  of  pulling  hair  and  flying  in  rude  play  at 
our  faces,  but  the  disposition  to  such  roughness  was  only  modified. 
A  little  broom  that  was  given  her  on  her  second  birthday  was 
used  to  strike  about  with  far  more  than  to  sweep,  for  a  few  weeks. 
In  the  latter  half  of  the  third  year  I  note  repeatedly  that  almost 
everything  the  child  has  to  be  checked  or  punished  for,  is  some 
ebullition  of  boisterousness,  or  rough,  unsympathetic  horse-play, 
—  shouting  and  kicking  her  feet  at  the  table,  banging  the  furniture 
with  sticks,  gayly  assaulting  us  or  the  cat  (her  roughness  to  whom 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  197 

became  a  constant  matter  of  discipline  in  the  thirty-fifth  month, 
and  on  into  the  fourth  year).  In  the  thirty-sixth  month,  finding  a 
handy,  smooth  stick,  she  struck  her  grandmother  sharply,  —  with- 
out the  least  temper,  merely  because  the  stick  was  tempting.  (This 
last  incident  called  so  pressingly  for  prompt  action  that  in  her 
mother's  absence  I  made  a  very  rare  exception  to  the  rule  never  to 
strike  another  person's  child,  and  turned  the  stick  against  herself. 
This  she  took  in  excellent  part  and  related  quite  approvingly  to  her 
mother  at  night  that  she  did  'pank  damma  wiv  a  'tick,  and  aunty 
had  to  'pank  her  wiv  de  'tik.)  By  the  end  of  the  year,  these 
roughnesses  were  mainly  checked,  except  toward  her  grandfather 
and  uncle,  who  rather  encouraged  them.1 

In  the  twenty-sixth,  twenty-seventh,  and  twenty-eighth  months, 
action  songs  were  a  good  deal  enjoyed:  "The  Barberry  Bush"  was 
liked  much  better  than  any  finger-songs;  and  in  this,  the  trunk 
and  leg  movements,  as  in  "'kipping  to  'cool,"  were  preferred  to  the 
hand  and  arm  movements. 

That  the  child  experienced  not  only  pleasure  but  more  or  less 

1  There  was  something  very  hard  to  understand  and  classify  in  this  rough 
play  impulse.  It  had  to  my  observation  something  peculiarly  primitive  and 
involuntary  about  it,  —  a  quality  that  mothers  recognize  when  they  say,  "The 
child  is  possessed  to  do  so  and  so."  There  was  an  element  of  cruelty  in  it, 
which  in  the  early  part  of  the  fourth  year  differentiated  itself  in  her  treatment 
of  the  cats;  there  was  a  horse-play  much  like  that  which  continues  so  strongly 
until  maturity  in  boys  if  their  training  is  neglected,  or  that  which  is  common  in 
the  lower  animals;  and  there  was  a  singularly  primitive  joy  in  the  sensation  of 
striking,  especially  when  suggested  by  a  stick  in  the  hands.  Thus  in  the 
thirty-ninth  month,  she  came  to  me  and  said  cheerfully,  "I  was  bad  to  Uncle 
Joe."  —  "Were  you,"  I  asked:  "Why?"  —  "O,  just  for  fun!  I  hit  him  with  a 
stick.  It  was  good  fun,  —  lots  of  fun."  There  was  some  bravado  in  this,  but 
I  did  not  doubt  she  had  stated  her  motive  truly  enough.  About  a  month  later, 
after  being  charmingly  good  for  a  long  time,  she  had  another  outbreak  of 
rowdyism,  and  while  her  grandfather  was  saying  grace  —  a  ceremony  which 
she  had  from  very  early  learned  to  treat  with  strict  reverence  — ■  she  went  and 
got  her  little  broom,  and  started  on  a  circuit  of  the  table,  dealing  a  thump  to 
each  bowed  head  in  turn,  till  seized;  when  she  remarked  with  enjoyment, 
"First  I  hit  papa,  then  I  hit  grandpa,  then  I  hit  grandma,  and  then  I  was  going 
to  hit  aunty." 

It  is  evident  that  the  subject  has  wide  relations,  both  in  animal  and  human 
psychology;  but  these  are  better  noted  under  some  category  of  moral  or  social 
development. 


198  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

intellectual  interest  in  muscular  sensation,  —  that  it  was  a  subject 
of  attention  to  her,  —  was  apparent  from  a  few  indications.  On 
the  forty-seventh  day,  she  showed  ludicrous  astonishment  at  a 
small,  high  crow  (the  first  she  had  ever  uttered),  which  had  sud- 
denly interpolated  itself  among  the  more  familiar  sounds  she  was 
making;  there  was  no  reason  from  her  general  behavior  about 
sounds  to  suppose  that  the  sound  alone,  without  the  novel  muscular 
sensation,  would  have  excited  such  attention.  In  this  same  week 
appeared  the  experimenting  with  the  tongue  —  putting  out  and 
withdrawing  the  tip  —  mentioned  above  (Sense  of  Contact,  p.  136). 
Experimenting  with  the  vocal  organs  in  the  fourth  month  has  also 
been  mentioned  (p.  189).  As  soon  as  she  was  four  months  old,  the 
baby  was  seated  daily  in  a  horse-collar  on  the  floor,  where  she 
could  sit  well  supported,  and  amuse  herself.  Within  a  week,  she 
began  bending  herself  back  over  the  collar.  The  sensation  (which 
was,  of  course,  partly  one  of  contact  and  partly  muscular;  perhaps 
sensations  of  equilibrium  were  also  involved)  seemed  to  wake  a 
special  curiosity  and  interest,  and  she  repeatedly  bent  her  body 
back  with  great  exertion ;  bent  it  so  far  that  the  back  of  her  head 
touched  the  floor.  This  touch  sensation,  which  amazed  her  greatly 
(see  Sense  of  Contact,  p.  143),  so  added  to  the  interest  of  bending 
back  that  she  kept  it  up  persistently  for  a  week  longer;  the  next 
day,  in  my  arms,  she  began  throwing  herself  back  over  my  arm, 
and  when  I  let  her  go  as  far  as  she  would,  holding  her  firmly,  she 
bent  back  with  a  serious  and  interested  face  till  her  head  was  hang- 
ing downward.  I  did  not  dare  let  her  persist  as  long  as  she  would, 
afraid  she  might  strain  herself.  In  the  following  days  she  would 
bend  back  till  her  head  rested  on  the  floor,  then  recover  herself 
Once  (135th  day  she  abandoned  the  breast  after  nursing  a  little, 
sat  up,  looked  at  me  with  a  broad  smile,  then  began  bending  herself 
back  over  her  mother's  arm. 

In  the  sixth  month  (163d  day)  the  baby's  grandmother  seated 
her  in  a  large  easy-chair,  where,  nestling  about  and  slipping  down, 
she  got  her  head  on  the  arm  of  the  chair.  She  then  stiffened  and 
raised  her  body  till  it  rested  on  her  heels  and  the  back  of  her  head. 
At  this  she  gave  a  squeal  of  delight;  then  relaxing  her  body,  she 
stiffened  it  again,  and  repeated  the  experiment  several  times.  (This 
incident  I  did  not  see  myself;  it  rests  on  the  grandmother's  report.) 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  199 

At  eleven  months  old,  the  baby  had  so  far  recognized  her  vari- 
ous actions  and  associated  them  with  words  as  to  be  able  to  obey 
twenty-seven  different  directions.  At  fifteen  months,  out  of  sixty- 
five  words  used  by  her,  seven,  or  perhaps  eight,  were  action  words. 
In  naming  these  actions,  of  course,  what  the  child  recognized  and 
named  was  partly  her  own  sensation  in  performing  them,  and  partly 
the  sight  of  herself  and  others  doing  so.  In  the  twentieth  month, 
"hard"  appeared,  in  the  expression  "too  hard,"  of  something  she 
thought  she  could  not  do.  In  the  twenty-second  I  find  it  as  an 
adverb,  "Ruth  pull  hard."  In  the  twenty-ninth  month,  I  find  this 
comment:  "This  is  a  hill.  We  walk  up.  That  is  pretty  hard. 
Oo-oop!  It  makes  me  say  'oo-oop/'"  "Rough,"  recorded  as  first 
used  in  the  twenty-first  month,  referred  to  roughness  discerned 
not  by  the  touch,  but  by  the  muscle  sense  in  walking  over  it; 
thus  in  the  twenty-second  month  the  child  apologized  as  follows 
for  asking  to  be  carried:  "Wu'  fai'  fa'  dowdn.  Gow  too  wii',"  — 
Ruth  fraid  fall  down;  ground  too  rough.  "Heavy"  I  find  used 
intelligently  from  the  twenty-second  month. 

Signs  of  fatigue  in  the  muscles  were  exceedingly  rare.  There 
may  have  been  an  element  of  muscular  sensation  in  the  weariness 
of  continued  position  spoken. of  below  (p.  208);  but  activity,  during 
the  whole  of  the  first  year,  seemed  incapable  of  tiring  the  baby.  A 
few  exceptions  to  this  statement  will  be  found  under  the  head  of 
Movements,  but  on  the  whole,  the  great  amount  of  exertion 
expended  during  the  first  year  in  learning  to  sit,  creep,  stand,  walk, 
etc.,  was  surprisingly  free  from  any  visible  effect  of  fatigue. 

During  the  second  and  third  years  also,  although  the  child  was 
incessantly  active,  my  notes  record  scarcely  any  instances  of  fatigue. 
The  most  marked  case  of  it  was  in  the  second  year,  at  eighteen 
months,  when  one  evening,  after  a  hard  romp  with  an  older  child, 
instead  of  being  as  usual  very  unwilling  to  stop  play,  she  came  and 
leaned  against  me,  and  when  I  took  her  in  my  lap,  asked  for  "bed," 
wanted  to  be  laid  in  her  crib  at  once,  and  nearly  cried  when  told 
she  must  be  undressed  first.  Had  she  had  other  children  to  play 
with,  especially  older  children,  she  doubtless  would  have  tired  her- 
self out  often.  At  the  beginning  of  the  twentieth  month,  she  once 
climbed   the  stairs  up  and   down,    refusing  to  stop,  until  she  had 


200  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

climbed  about  160  steps  in  all;  toward  the  end  she  was  evidently- 
very  tired,  and  her  feet  dragged,  though  she  persisted.  I  find  men- 
tion several  times  in  the  second  and  third  years  that  she  played  till 
she  was  "hot  and  tired,"  or  "laughed  till  she  was  tired,"  and  some- 
times that  she  asked  to  be  carried  on  rough  ground,  or  after  walk- 
ing a  good  deal. 

The  word  "rest"  was  used  as  early  as  the  seventeenth  month, 
(she  would  sit  down  to  "rest"  when  climbing  the  stairs,  or  on  a 
stone  or  box  when  outdoors);  and  "tired"  by  the  nineteenth.  In 
the  twenty-fourth  month,  she  would  say,  "Ruth  so  tired!"  flinging 
herself  down;  and  once:  "You  tired.  You  been  outdoors,  so  you 
lay  down."  It  is  doubtful,  however,  whether  she  used  either  word 
in  any  but  an  imitative  way  within  the  first  three  years. 

2.   Sensation  of  Motion. 

The  probability  is  that  sensation  of  motion  exists  from  the  first, 
and  that  had  I  watched  for  it,  I  might  have  detected  evidence  of  it 
in  the  form  of  some  change  in  the  baby's  expression  or  behavior 
when  she  was  moved;  but  as  she  was  carried  about  as  little  as  pos- 
sible in  the  first  month,  and  merely  lifting  her  from  one  place  to 
another  affected  the  sense  of  position  even  more  than  that  of  motion, 
the  opportunity  for  such  an  observation  was  slight.  The  only 
notes  that  I  have  concerning  this  sensation  are  notes  of  the  pleasure 
afforded  by  it;  and  as  will  be  seen,  in  most  cases,  the  sources  of  the 
pleasure  were  not  simply  the  sensation  of  motion,  which  soon  be- 
came blent  with  other  sensations,  especially  muscular  ones.  On 
the  whole,  however,  it  was  true  that  during  the  first  half-year  the 
pleasure  given  by  passive  motion  exceeded  that  of  muscular  activity. 
With  the  child's  acquirement  of  power  over  her  own  movements, 
this  relation  was  reversed. 

My  first  note  of  any  indication  of  pleasure  in  motion  (viz.,  that 
the  first  time  the  baby  was  carried  about  outdoors,  on  the  twenty- 
ninth  day,  she  showed  a  pleasure  that  must  have  been  due  either  to 
the  fresh  air  or  the  motion,  since  the  brighter  light  seemed 
unpleasant  to  her)  precedes  by  about  two  months  my  first  note  of 
pleasure  in  active  movement.  Enjoyment  of  being  wheeled  about 
in  a  baby-carriage  was  evident  from  the  first  time  she  was  taken  out 
thus  (thirty-eighth  day).     That  the  pleasure  was  mainly  a  sort  of 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  201 

tranquil  comfort  in  the  feeling  of  motion,  was  evident  from  the 
baby's  behavior:  she  did  not  smile  or  crow,  but  lay  with  a  blissful 
expression  as  long  as  the  motion  continued,  and  soon  began  to  fret 
if  it  stopped.  For  weeks  after  she  was  constantly  desirous  of  sitting 
up  indoors,  she  was  content  to  lie  down  in  the  baby-carriage,  as 
long  as  it  was  moving.  In  the  fourth  month,  though  indoors  she 
was  occupied  in  gazing  at  things  with  curiosity,  when  wheeled 
about  outdoors,  she  paid  no  attention  to  the  objects  about  her,  but 
lay  tranquilly  looking  before  her,  seeming  to  care  only  for  the 
motion.  The  swifter  motion  of  a  drive  behind  horses  gave  a  keener 
pleasure,  but  still  tranquil  in  quality:  on  her  first  drive  (105th  day) 
she  did  not  look  about  her  at  all,  and  remained  perfectly  silent  till 
we  had  driven  about  half  a  mile,  then  uttered  one  long  murmur  of 
exceeding  content,  and  became  silent  again  for  the  rest  of  the  drive. 
In  the  last  week  of  the  fourth  month,  a  change  appeared:  one  day 
(the  119th)  instead  of  lying  in  still  satisfaction,  the  baby  began  to 
utter  happy  murmurs  as  she  was  wheeled  about,  and  presently 
fretted  to  sit  up;  leaned  forward  to  watch  me  pluck  something  (the 
first  time  she  had  shown  interest  in  the  use  of  her  eyes  outdoors); 
and  three  days  later  insisted  on  sitting  up  and  looking  about. 
From  this  time  her  pleasure  in  being  wheeled  about,  and  still  more 
in  driving,  became  more  complex  and  keener;  in  the  eleventh  month 
the  mere  sight  of  her  baby-carriage  excited  the  liveliest  demonstra- 
tions of  joy  and  desire. 

Meanwhile,  in  the  seventh  month  (202d  day)  she  showed  interest 
and  pleasure  but  not  gayety  at  being  swung  in  a  blanket.  In  the 
ninth  month,  and  again  in  the  tenth  and  eleventh,  I  find  notes  of 
marked  pleasure  at  being  swung  in  a  hammock:  though  usually  so 
active  and  restless,  she  would  lie  still  and  smiling  on  my  arm  in  the 
hammock,  or  sit  quietly  beside  me.  Two  or  three  times  in  the 
ninth  and  eleventh  months  I  put  her  into  a  basket  and  carried  it 
about,  or  swung  it  by  the  handle,  to  her  great  pleasure.  An  excep- 
tion to  the  serenity  of  her  pleasure  in  being  carried  about  with  a 
fairly  even  motion,  was  her  laughter  and  glee  when  some  one  set 
her  on  a  carpet-sweeper  and  trundled  her  about;  but  a  considerable 
element  of  amusing  novelty  doubtless  entered  into  this  pleasure, 
and  she  showed  much  the  same  glee  when  she  saw  another  child  on 
the  sweeper  as  when  she  rode  on  it  herself. 


202  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

The  influence  of  motion  in  inducing  sleep  is  one  of  the 
commonest  nursery  observations.  My  notes  on  this  point  I  reserve 
for  the  subject  of  Sleep.  I  have  already  referred,  in  speaking  of 
Rhythm,  under  the  head  of  Hearing  (p.  119)  to  the  superior  quiet- 
ing influence  of  a  monotonous  jarring  as  compared  to  smooth 
motion.  It  is  frequently  said  that  trotting  and  jolting  babies  stops 
their  fretting  only  by  diversion  of  attention,  and  this  is  doubtless 
true  in  some  cases ;  but  there  were  many  instances  in  which  my 
niece's  behavior  showed  that  there  was  something  agreeable  and 
quieting  in  the  motion  itself.  Thus  on  the  forty-eighth  day  her 
fretting  stopped  and  her  discontented  expression  changed  to  a 
happy  one  the  instant  her  grandmother,  who  held  her  on  her  knee, 
began  to  trot  her.  At  this  time  (second  month)  if  she  chanced  to 
be  fretting  in  the  baby-carriage  as  it  rolled  over  the  smooth  veranda, 
she  stopped  and  became  placid  the  instant  it  struck  the  gravel  walk. 
In  the  sixth  month,  it  was  once  necessary  to  carry  her  a  block  or 
two  through  the  rain,  so  covering  her,  head  and  all,  with  my  cloak,  I 
ran  with  her.  She  had  begun  to  protest  at  being  thus  muffled,  but 
stopped  at  the  motion,  and  came  out  smiling  when  I  reached  shelter. 
Later  the  same  day,  when  a  series  of  misadventures  had  left  her 
grievously  tired  and  hungry,  her  nap  broken,  her  mother  detained 
past  nursing  time,  I  ran  with  her  a  block  or  two  to  catch  a  train; 
and  she  forgot  her  troubles  at  once,  and  stopped  crying  as  long  as 
the  motion  lasted. 

At  three  months  old,  she  liked  to  be  tossed  in  her  father's  arms, 
and  during  the  fourth  month  became  very  fond  of  a  frolic,  and 
would  crow  and  smile  in  high  glee  when  she  was  tossed  in  the  air, 
slid  down  one's  knees,  or  otherwise  tumbled  about;  the  first  true 
laughter  I  heard  from  her  was  over  such  a  frolic  in  the  last  week  of 
this  month  (1 1 8th  day);  and  in  the  first  six  months  this  was  almost 
the  only  cause  of  laughter.  In  the  fifth  month  (nineteenth  week)  she 
began  to  coax  to  be  played  with,  with  motions  and  cries  of  desire; 
my  notes  of  this  coaxing  and  of  her  laughter  and  delight  in  the 
play,  are  very  frequent  in  the  next  three  months,  —  for  weeks 
together,  almost  daily.  Thus  on  the  133d  day,  seated  on  her  mother's 
foot  and  danced  up    and   down  (held  by  the  arms),  she  wore  an 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  2  03 

expression  of  rapt  delight,  and  whenever  her  mother  stopped  she 
would  set  up  a  little  cry  of  desire.     That  evening  her  father  played 
with  her  at  bedtime,  and  the  next  night  at  the  same  hour  she  whim- 
pered for  a  play  instead  of  settling  down  to  sleep.     The  first  recog- 
nition of  her  father  that  she  showed  was  a  month  later  (160th  day) 
after  he  had  tossed  and  jumped  her  a  great  deal;  that  evening  when 
he  came  into  the  room  she  showed  expectation  of  a  romp,  looking 
at  him  with  laughter  and  movements  of  her  arms,  and  for  some 
days  after,  she  repeated  this  whenever  he  appeared.     In  the  same 
week,  the  twenty-third,  I  took  her  in  my  arms  and  jumped  up  and 
down  with  her,  to  her  great  delight.     This  became  a  favorite  play, 
and  she  would  beg  for  more  with  cries  of  desire.     The  day  she  was 
.twenty-four  weeks  old,  as  soon  as  she  saw  me  in  the  morning  she 
began  making  little  wistful  cries,  with  an  earnest  look  into  my  face 
and  motions  of  the  body;  and  continued  to  show  signs  of  desire  for 
half  an  hour,  till  I  gratified  her.     Three  days  later,  after  a  frolic  of 
the  sort,  I  put  her  down  in  her  mother's  lap:  she  looked  up, laugh- 
ing and  springing  with  her  body,  but  when  I  left  the  room  she 
stared  after  me  with  an  expression  of  surprise  and  grief  (as  I  saw 
through  a  crack  of  the  door).     I  opened  the  door  and  spoke,  and 
she  broke  into  smiles  and  movements  again.     This  was   repeated 
several  times;  then  I  remained  out  of  sight.     She  looked  sober  for 
about  ten  seconds;  then,  with  her  usual  easy  surrender  of  desire, 
looked  up  to  her  mother's  face  and  laughed. 

In  the  twenty-fourth  week  her  father  began  another  play  that 
was  very  delightful  to  her,  —  swinging  or  tossing  her  into  her 
mother's  arms,  or  mine,  to  be  swung  back  into  his;  sometimes  the 
three  of  us  passed  her  thus  from  one  to  another.  This  excited  great 
hilarity;  she  reached  her  arms  from  one  to  another  and  laughed 
aloud;  and  when  the  frolic  was  stopped  and  she  was  taken  from 
the  room,  she  set  up  a  remonstrant  whine.  I  have  a  number 
of  notes  afterward  of  merriment  over  this  play  and  desire  for  it. 

In  two  or  three  cases  at  least,  she  knew  very  well  which  play 
she  wanted,  and  when  her  coaxing  sounds  and  outstretched  hands 
had  persuaded  one  of  us  to  take  her  for  a  romp,  she  looked  only 
moderately  satisfied  until  the  right  play  was  hit  on. 

In  the  second  half-year,  her  love  of  this  passive  play  continued 
for  a  time.     During  the  seventh  and  eighth  months,  it  was  quite 


204  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

common  for  her  to  fret  and  coax  for  a  frolic  in  the  evening,  instead 
of  going  to  sleep ;  and  in  the  eighth  month  her  desire  to  go  to 
people  was  almost  entirely  according  as  they  would  frolic  with  her, 
— ■  though  by  the  end  of  the  eighth  month  she  had  made  distinc- 
tions, and  while  she  had  no  use  for  her  father  unless  he  would  toss 
and  play  with  her,  she  was  happy  with  me  without  the  playing. 
After  this  her  great  desire  for  such  play  disappeared,  displaced  by 
her  growing  desire  for  self-activity :  though  I  find  a  few  notes  in  the 
tenth  and  eleventh  months  of  laughter  and  delight  in  being  rolled 
and  tumbled  about,  or  tossed  from  one  person  to  another.  Once  at 
forty-four  weeks  old,  she  was  very  happy  over  being  walked  with 
rapidly,  and  for  many  seconds  uttered  a  crow  with  each  inhalation 
and  a  joyous  murmur  with  each  exhalation.  At  another  time, 
when  just  forty-seven  weeks  old,  she  tried  repeatedly  to  creep  to  a 
pitcher  that  stood  on  the  floor,  and  each  time  I  pulled  her  back  by 
her  dress  and  rolled  her  over  on  the  floor,  a  rough  treatment  that 
was  received  with  peals  of  laughter  instead  of  displeasure. 

In  the  sixth  month,  I  had  once  chanced  to  amuse  her  by  joun- 
cing her  up  and  down  on  the  springs  of  a  bed,  a  motion  that  pleased 
her  greatly:  in  the  tenth  month  this  became  a  favorite  play,  con- 
stantly mentioned  in  my  notes.  She  liked  to  have  the  jouncing 
violent  enough  to  fling  her  about,  and  liked  best  to  take  it  lying  on 
her  face,  so  that  her  face  was  thumped  roughly  into  the  mattress, 
at  which  she  would  laugh  aloud  with  delight;  but  it  pleased  her 
greatly  also  to  be  flung  off  her  balance  when  sitting,  or  on  her  hands 
and  knees.  If  I  stopped,  she  would  wait  a  few  seconds,  then  indi- 
cate desire  to  go  on  by  springing  movements  of  her  body,  or  by 
rolling  over  with  her  face  flat  on  the  mattress  and  making  little  mur- 
murs of  desire.  The  259th  day,  after  I  had  carried  her  into  another 
room  and  put  her  down,  she  sat  and  looked  earnestly  at  me  with 
urgent  chatter,  an  unmistakable  appeal  to  be  taken  back  to  go  on 
with  the  play. 

In  these  plays  it  is  evident  that  the  simple  sensation  of  motion 
was  more  and  more  mingled  with  other  sensations,  those  of  touch, 
position,  equilibrium,  even  of  muscular  activity,  from  the  involuntary 
flinging  about  of  limbs  and  balancing  of  the  body.  The  pleasure  of 
muscular  action  and  of  passive  motion  were  almost  equally  mingled  in 
jouncing  up  and  down  to  rock  the  baby-carriage  on  its  springs,  or 


Shinn]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  -OJ 

sitting  in  a  little  rocking-chair  and  making  it  go  by  •movements 
of  her  body  (at  first  jiggling  it  awkwardly,  but  by  the  forty-fourth 
week  rocking  it  quite  nicely,  though  her  legs  were  too  short  to 
hang  down  over  the  edge  of  the  seat)  —  employments  that  were 
much  enjoyed  from  time  to  time  from  the  thirty-second  week  to  the 
end  of  the  year.  In  the  twelfth  month  she  would  persistently 
climb  to  her  feet  in  the  baby-carriage,  to  rock  it  from  side  to  side, 
at  imminent  risk  of  falling. 

On  the  other  hand,  in  the  middle  of  the  twelfth  month  (350th 
day)  she  laughed  very  much  over  a  play  in  which  there  was  no 
muscular  element:  merely  being  sent  back  and  forth  in  the  baby- 
carriage  between  her  mother  and  me,  from  end  to  end  of  the 
veranda,  by  a  push  that  sent  it  safely  along  the  smooth  pavement;  then 
when  her  mother  began  to  roll  the  carriage  and  reverse  its  motion 
with  a  jerk,  keeping  time  to  the  motion  with  rhythmic  sounds,  the 
baby  was  enchanted;  she  would  begin  to  laugh  in  joyous  expecta- 
tion of  each  jolt  as  she  perceived  it  approaching.1 

'The  main  pleasure  of  such  a  play  is  certainly  in  the  arrangement  of  the 
motion  and  sounds,  —  a  rhythmic  series,  ending  in  a  shock;  all  babies,  so  far  as 
I  have  seen,  after  the  first  six  months,  are  amused  by  rhythmic  motions  or 
sounds  (better  both  joined)  ending  in  an  explosion,  or  sudden  climax;  as  one 
may  see  by  simply  remarking  to  a  baby,  "Bobby-bob,  bobby-bob,  BOB,"  with 
accompanying  demonstrations  of  head  and  hands,  — or  taking  the  little  hands 
and  patting  them  together  some  half  dozen  times,  ending  with  a  clap,  —  or  any 
one  of  fifty  like  exercises,  familiar  to  every  nursery.  If  the  baby  is  a  merry 
one  and  not  timid,  such  experiments  will  be  rewarded  with  peals  of  laughter 
and  pleas  to  continue,  till  the  experimenter  is  thoroughly  bored  with  his  own 
performance.  But  the  forms  in  which  the  rhythm  and  concluding  jar  are  most 
appreciated  by  the  baby  are  those  that  affect  his  own  body;  a  series  of  threats 
ending  with  ducking  the  head  into  his  bosom  is  more  amusing  than  one  that 
ends  in  a  mere  hand-clap  (or  vide  the  favorite  play,  "Creep,  Mousey,  Crawl  "); 
and  there  were  several  instances  in  the  case  of  my  niece  besides  the  above 
(see  self-invented  plays  under  Movement  below;  and  the  sliding  downstairs, 
p.  207  below)  which  show  that  a  heavy  jar  of  the  whole  body  was  a  particularly 
interesting  form  of  climax. 

It  seems  to  me  clear,  also,  that  healthy  children  experience  little  of  the 
unpleasant  sensation  from  jar  that  their  elders  do.  At  eight  or  nine  years  old  I 
used  to  play,  with  several  other  children,  at  finding  our  way  with  shut  eyes  in 
a  meadow  that  broke  off  into  gullies  with  banks  some  two  to  five  feet  high; 
and  we  thought  it  much  better  fun  to  miss  our  way  and  plunge  over  these 
banks  than  to  reach  our  goal  safely.  If  I  remember  right,  some  headaches 
resulted  from  going  over  the  highest  places,  which  led  to  the  play's  being  dis- 


206  University  of  California.  Ivol.  i. 

In  the  second  year  pleasure  in  mere  passive  motion  declined,  as 
the  child's  power  over  her  own  body  increased;  and  in  the  nineteenth 
and  twentieth  months,  she  became  positively  averse  to  vehicles 
thus,  in  the  eighty-seventh  week,  I  note  that  she  was  disposed  to 
whimper  when  she  heard  something  said  about  driving,  as  her 
bonnet  was  put  on,  and  begged,  "Walk  !  walk  outdoors  ! "  and  in  the 
ninety-first  week,  that  she  was  borne  off  to  drive,  protesting  lamenta- 
bly that  she  wanted  to  "walk  on  feet."  There  "are  also  many  notes 
of  unwillingness  to  be  carried.  This  lasted  during  the  whole  half- 
year,  but  with  the  third  year  she  became  first  reconciled  to  driving, 
then  moderately  fond  of  it ;  and  in  the  twenty-seventh  month,  though 
impatient  of  sitting  still  in  any  one's  lap  for  a  chat,  she  liked  to  have 
me  take  her  in  my  arms  and  walk  about,  chatting  with  her.  In  the 
thirty-second  month,  she  was  greatly  pleased  at  being  set  on  the 
seat  of  a  bicycle,  and  wheeled  about. 

I  find  some  note  of  liking  for  the  hammock  in  the  second  year 
A  little  swing  was  given  her  on  her  second  birthday,  and  swinging 
at  once  became  a  favorite  amusement ;  by  the  thirtieth  month,  she 
wanted  to  be  sent  as  high  as  the  ropes  (over  four  feet  long)  would 
take  her;  in  the  thirty-third  month  especially,  she  begged  daily  to 
be  swung,  and  swung  high.  She  has  had,  in  her  sixth  and  seventh 
years,  a  swing  with  ropes  eighteen  feet  long,  and  likes  to  be  swung 
as  high  as  the  stoutest  masculine  arms  can  send  her. 

During  the  first  half  of  the  second  year,  I  find  a  few  notes  of  her 
continued  pleasure  in  being  jounced  on  mattress  springs,  but  the  play 
was  probably  stopped  as  she  grew  rather  heavy  for  the  springs. 

She  continued  in  this  half-year  to  like  rocking  in  a  chair,  but 
now  with  a  smooth  and  gentle  motion;  and  to  be  tossed,  swung, 
and  rolled  about  (as,  indeed,  she  does  still).  In  the  sixteenth  month 
she  was  deeply  delighted  when  a  guest  took  her  in  her  arms  and 
danced  with  her,  —  an  entertainment  she  had  never  had  before, — 
and  was  hardly  willing  to  let  this  guest  out  of  sight  the  rest  of  the 
day;  the  incident  made  an  unusual  impression  on  her  memory,  also. 
Such  plays  as  "Shoe  the  horse,"  "This  is  the  way  the  ladies  ride," 

covered  and  stopped;  but  when  the  bank  was  not  more  than  two  or  three  feet 
high,  I  remember  the  fall  and  jar  as  an  exciting  pleasure. 

On  the  other  hand,  an  unexpected  fall  and  jar  could  frighten  my  niece  very 
much. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  20J 

"Trim-tram,"  "Ride  away  to  Boston  town,"  gave  pleasure;  in  "Bos- 
ton town"  (seventeenth  month)  she  would  begin  to  smile  in  antici- 
pation as  the  climax,  "fall  down,"  approached.1  In  the  seventy- 
second  week,  her  father  placed  her  on  the  footboard  of  the  bed,  and 
repeating,  "Humpty  Dumpty,"  tipped  her  over  on  the  bed  with  the 
word  "fall."  She  was  delighted  with  this  and  wished  it  repeated  a 
dozen  times.  In  the  twenty-first  month,  in  a  cable  car  where  we 
were  the  only  passengers,  she  insisted  on  walking  back  and  forth 
on  the  empty  seat  the  length  of  the  car,  enjoying  the  way  she  was 
flung  about  by  the  jerking  of  the  car  (guarded  by  me,  of  course, 
from  falling).  At  twenty-one  months  old,  she  liked  to  go  down- 
stairs, held  by  some  one's  hands,  sliding  on  her  feet  from  step  to 
step  with  a  racking  jolt.  In  the  twenty-eighth  month,  when  traveling 
on  the  cars,  her  favorite  occupation  was  to  climb  up  on  the  window- 
ledge,  and  fling  herself  backward  from  it  into  our  laps  with  entire 
abandon. 

An  incident  of  the  eighteenth  month  (535th  day)  showed  an 
ability  to  imagine  and  desire  (though  doubtless  most  imperfectly)  a 
joy  of  motion  outside  her  experience.  She  had  all  day  been  running 
toward  the  birds  that  lighted  on  the  ground,  and  saying  "Catch  ! "  At 
last,  when  they  flew  up,  she  put  up  one  little  hand,  saying  "F'y!" 
in  a  tone  of  desire.  "Yes,  the  birdies  did  fly,"  I  said;  but  she  re- 
peated longingly,  "  Wu' !  Wu' !  F'y  !  "  "  Does  Ruth  want  to  fly  ?  " 
I  asked.  "E"  (yes).  I  took  her  and  tossed  her  up  into  the  air,  which 
seemed  rather  to  divert  her  mind  than  to  satisfy  her  desire. 

The  words  fast  and  slow  were  used  from  the  eighteenth  month, 
in  a  very  limited  way,  —  of  her  own  pace  in  walking. 

3.  Sensations  of  Position. 

The  complex  sensation  of  position  seemed  to  me  to  form  itself 
early,  since  by  the  fourth  week  a  certain  position  was  quite 
distinctly  associated  with  the  satisfaction  of  hunger,  —  long  before 
any  sight  association  with  the  breast  could  be  detected.  Associa- 
tions of  position   with    sleep  also    were  quickly  formed.     I   shall 

1  Ride  away,  ride  away,  to  Boston  town, 
But  take  care,  my  little  girl,  that  you  don't  —  fall  — down!" 
With  the  last  words,  the  child  is  tipped  backward  from  the  knee,  held  by 
the  hands  from  falling. 


208  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

speak  of  these  associations  under  the  heads  of  Hunger,  and  Sleep, 
however.  From  the  middle  of  the  second  month,  the  baby  had 
favorite  positions,  which  she  greatly  preferred  to  ordinary  lying 
down :  one  a  position  in  which  her  grandmother  held  her,  almost 
sitting  up;  another  lying  along  the  knees,  feet  against  the  body  of 
the  one  who  held  her,  and  head  in  the  extended  palm;  another 
(third  and  fourth  months),  lying  up  against  the  shoulder.  The 
desire  to  look  about  her  had  much  to  do  with  her  liking  for  the 
erect  positions,  but  she  evidently  found  them  very  comfortable 
also;  on  the  knees,  or  against  the  shoulder,  she  would  readily  go 
to  sleep.     (See  note,  opposite  page.) 

Sensations  of  fatigue,  due  to  continuance  in  one  position,  were 
early  apparent.  From  the  first  the  nurse  took  such  fatigue  into 
account  as  among  the  regular  causes  of  restlessness,  and  would 
change  the  baby's  position  when  she  was  fretting  and  no  other 
reason  for  discomfort  appeared;  and  as  a  rule  the  fretting  would 
cease.  Until  the  baby  became  able  to  change  her  position  herself, 
weariness  was  regularly  announced  by  fretting  sounds,  which  by 
the  fourth  month  would  rise  to  a  whimper,  and  threaten  a  positive 
wail  if  she  was  not  moved.  When  she  had  learned  to  turn  over, 
she  always  did  so  before  long  if  laid  down  on  her  back  (latter 
part  of  fifth  month);  but  soon  wearied  of  the  new  position  and 
fretted  for  relief;  when  she  could  roll  over  and  back  again,  how- 
ever (seventh  and  eighth  months),  she  was  happy  by  the  hour, 
rolling  and  kicking  on  the  floor  or  lawn,  without  desiring  the  least 
attention. 

I  have  also  a  number  of  notes,  from  the  end  of  the  fourth 
month  on,  of  her  getting  tired,  after  twenty  minutes  or  so,  of 
sitting  in  her  high-chair,  or  any  similarly  restricted  seat,  and 
fretting  to  be  taken  from  it,  when  she  would  at  once  become 
jolly.  In  this,  however,  there  was  probably  not  as  much  real 
fatigue  from  continued  position  as  mental  restlessness,  and  dis- 
content from  the  prohibition  of  active  movement. 

The  weariness  from  continued  position,  which  was  unmistak- 
able during  the  first  year,  and  in  a  less  degree  throughout  the 
second  and  third,  was  evidently  a  complex  sensation,  in  which  the 
vaguely  unpleasant  feelings  arising  from  impeded  circulation  are 
likely  to  have  made  up  the  principal   part.     Probably  there   was 


Shinn]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  209 

also  some  muscular  fatigue  from  the  continuance  of  one  set  of 
contractions,  —  though  considering  the  relaxed  condition  in  which 
a  young  baby  lies,  this  must  have  been  in  the  earliest  weeks,  an 
unimportant  element,  if  it  existed  then  at  all.  Pressure  from  the 
weight  of  the  body  on  the  muscles  and  skin  would  have  more 
share  in  the  discomfort.1  We  ourselves  experience  numberless 
incipient  sensations  of  weariness,  pressure,  and  restraint,  and 
relieve  them,  scarcely  consciously,  by  all  manner  of  slight 
changes  of  position,  general  or  local.  The  entire  helplessness  of 
the  young  baby  thus  to  relieve  them,  even  after  they  become 
severe,  is  not  sufficiently  remembered;  and  this  neglect  doubtless 
accounts  for  much  mysterious  fretfulness.  It  is  an  old  practice  of 
careful  mothers  to  change  the  position  of  a  baby  at  intervals 
during  long-continued  sleep;  partly  because  they  believe  it  affords 
relief,  and  makes  sleep  easier,  and  partly  to  prevent  fixed  habits 
of  position  that  will  make  growth  unsymmetrical. 

4.    Sensations  of  Equilibrium. 

Equilibrium  sensations  must  have  accompanied  the  whole  pro- 
cess of  acquiring  the  balance  of  head  and  body,  and  this  began  in 
the  first  month;  but  until  its  later  stages,  (learning  to  stand  and  walk,) 
the  child's  manner  did  not  show  consciousness  of  them.  In  these 
later  stages  feelings  of  insecurity,  and  pleasure  in  feelings  of  assured 
equilibrium,  were  abundantly  shown.  In  being  carried  about  in  the 
arms  of  others,  in  being  tossed,  etc.,  she  showed  no  sign  that  I 
could  detect  of  such  feelings;  though  they  may  have  formed  part  of 
the  pleasure  of  being  tossed,  swung,  etc.,  as  we  ourselves  experience 
them  in  swinging,  sliding,  wheeling,  and  the  like  exercises.  See 
also  on  p.  198  above. 

I  never  saw  any  sign  of  a  sensation  of  giddiness;  neither  swing- 
ing, rocking  in  the  hammock,  nor  being  tossed  and  swung  in  all 
manner  of  ways  in  people's  arms,  seemed  able  to  cause  it.     Up  to 

1  The  extent  to  which  we  feel  the  weight  of  the  abdominal  viscera  alone 
can  be  realized  from  the  curious  sense  of  relief  given  by  certain  gymnasium 
exercises,  in  which  the  weight  of  these  viscera  is  taken  from  the  muscles  that 
usually  bear  it,  and  thrown  upon  the  diaphragm.  A  change  in  the  direction 
of  the  visceral  weight  probably  had  to  do  with  the  pleasure  in  being  held 
erect. 


2  I O  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

seven  years  old,  she  has  no  knowledge  of  the  sensation  except  when 
she  deliberately  produces  it  by  spinning  round  and  round,  or  by 
twisting  her  swing  ropes  and  allowing  them  to  untwist.  She  does 
not  find  the  sensation  disagreeable,  and  seeks  it  for  amusement; 
and  this  is,  I  think,  the  rule  with  little  children.  Dizziness  seems 
to  be  less  easily  produced  in  them,  and  to  be  a  less  disagreeable 
sensation  than  with  us.  It  is  unusual  to  find  a  child  that  is 
made  dizzy  by  a  swing;  and  little  children  love  to  get  into  an  old- 
fashioned  cradle,  or  anything  that  will  tilt  to  and  fro,  and  to  rock 
themselves  by  the  hour  with  considerable  violence,  playing  at  ship 
or  stage.  All  the  children  that  I  played  with  as  a  child  used  to 
spin  about  to  produce  dizziness,  and  amuse  themselves  with  the 
stumbling  about  afterward,  and  I  do  not  remember  finding  it  dis- 
agreeable. 


ORGANIC    SENSATIONS. 

i.  Hunger  and  Satiety. 

Although  the  baby  seemed  to  require  scarcely  any  nourishment 
at  the  first,  and  for  a  period  whose  length  I  have  failed  to  record, 
(certainly  more  than  forty-eight  hours,)  seemed  comfortable  and 
well  without  food,  yet  some  sensation  of  hunger  or  thirst,  which 
it  required  only  a  little  warm  sweetened  water  to  satisfy,  was  shown 
by  signs  that  the  nurse  could  recognize.  As  soon  as  placed  at  the 
breast  she  sucked  strongly;  and  in  the  following  days  there  was 
some  little  crying  from  hunger,  which  the  nurse  seemed  able  to  dis- 
tinguish quite  certainly  from  crying  for  colic,  though  I  was  not 
able  to  do  so. 

Until  nearly  the  end  of  the  tenth  week,  the  interval  between  meals 
was  two  hours,  but  hunger  appeared  somewhat  irregularly,  often 
some  little  time  before  the  expiration  of  the  interval;  once  (50th 
day)  the  baby  was  fiercely  hungry  in  an  hour  and  a  half.  (In  this  case 
she  had  been  awake,  bathed,  talked  to,  and  played  with  during  the 
whole  time,  though  without  showing  any  excitement.  I  had  an 
impression  that  as  a  rule  hunger  was  hastened  by  an  excess  of 
occupation  for  the  brain  and  senses,  but  I  did  not  see  any  distinct 
evidence  of  this  except  in  the  one  instance;  the  conditions  were 
probably  too  complex.  Conversely,  it  was  certainly  true  that  sleep 
delayed  hunger.)  On  the  76th  day  she  refused  to  take  the 
breast  until  four  hours  and  a  half  had  passed,  and  after  that  would 
not  take  it  at  a  less  interval  than  two  hours  and  a  half,  so  this  be- 
came the  regular  period.  By  the  fifteenth  week  the  baby  some- 
times positively  refused  to  nurse  at  this  interval,  turning  her  head 
away  and  holding  it  so  rigidly  that  it  could  scarcely  be  forced  to 
the  breast.  She  would  then  become  hungry  about  half  an  hour 
later;  so  in  the  sixteenth  week  the  interval  was  extended  to  three 
hours.  In  the  first  week  of  the  seventh  month,  188th  day,  she  re- 
fused to  eat  at  the  interval  of  three  hours;  the  next  day  at  three 
hours  and  a  half;  two  weeks  later,  she  often  refused  to  eat  till  four 
hours  had  passed,  though  sometimes  she  insisted  on  food  in  two 
and  a  half  hours. 

15  <2II> 


212  University  of  California.  Vol.  i. 

In  the  first  two  months,  when  hunger  was  apt  to  anticipate  the 
two-hour  interval,  it  was  not  thought  best  to  break  much  into  the 
regularity  of  feeding,  and  the  baby  was  sometimes  kept  waiting  ten 
or  fifteen  minutes  after  she  was  hungry.  At  these  times  hunger 
seemed  to  come  on  her  suddenly,  her  pleasant  mood  changing  at 
once  into  fretting,  without  any  intervening  gradations;  then  if  waiting 
was  prolonged,  the  little  discontented  noises  would  rise  to  crying, 
sometimes  loud  and  imperative,  sometimes  a  sort  of  complaining  cry 
that  rose  to  wails  and  sank  again  to  murmurs.  On  the  45th  day  I 
find  moving  the  arms  and  pursing  the  lips  as  in  sucking  mentioned 
as  signs  of  hunger,  and  for  about  two  months  afterward  sucking 
movements  and  sometimes  smacking  sounds  were  regular  signs  of 
moderate  hunger;  sometimes  her  fist,  accidentally  brought  to  her 
mouth  when  she  was  hungry,  was  sucked.  If  her  face  was  held  up 
to  one's  cheek  when  she  was  hungry,  she  would  lay  hold  on  the 
cheek  with  her  lips  and  suck  it  frantically.  In  the  fourth  month 
pursing  the  lips,  sucking,  and  smacking,  became  habits  without  any 
connection  with  hunger;  in  the  sixth  month  I  thought  smack- 
ing denoted  hunger,  but  it  was  not  done  regularly  enough  to  make 
me  sure  of  this.  On  the  46th  day  I  mention  a  doubling  and  stiffen- 
ing of  the  body  while  crying  with  hunger.  After  the  baby  had 
begun  herself  to  extend  the  interval  of  nursing,  it  was  rarely  that 
hunger  anticipated  the  offering  of  the  breast,  and  when  it  did  it  was 
only  in  a  moderate  degree;  I  find  but  a  single  record  of  crying  for 
hunger,  and  only  now  and  then  of  fretfulness  and  complaining 
noises.  In  the  sixth  month  when  the  baby  was  taken  on  her 
mother's  lap  and  unbuttoning  the  dress  began,  these  sounds  would 
pass  into  an  excited  whimpering,  with  tugging  at  the  dress.  Once 
in  the  ninth  month  I  note  that  she  cried  for  the  breast  when  the 
time  came  and  nursing  was  for  some  reason  postponed  after  her 
mother  had  once  taken  her:  I  mention  expressly  that  it  was  rare  for 
her  to  seem  thus  eager  and  hungry. 

Sensibility  to  disagreeable  impressions  was  heightened  by  hunger : 
to  be  dressed  while  hungry,  e.  g.,  was  a  great  grievance.  While 
hunger  was  being  satisfied,  on  the  other  hand,  sensibility  was 
lowered,  if  I  may  generalize  from  the  experiments  recorded  under 
Hearing,  23d  day  (p.  107).  Yet  once  while  nursing  she  was  con- 
siderably startled  by  a  chunk  of  coal  dropped  on  the  hearth,  (134th 
day,  see  Hearing,  p.  108.) 


Shins.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  213 

It  seemed  evidence  of  the  importance  of  the  sensation  that  the 
satisfaction  of  hunger  (together  with  a  sensation  of  position)  made 
the  material  of  the  first  distinct  association,  at  three  weeks  old.  I 
noticed  at  this  time  that  when  the  baby  was  crying  with  hunger, 
she  would  at  once  become  quiet  on  being  lifted  in  the  arms  in  the 
position  usual  in  nursing,  or  laid  on  the  bed  beside  her  mother, 
where  she  was  often  nursed.  By  the  seventh  week,  when  crying 
loudly  with  hunger,  she  would  stop  as  soon  as  she  was  lifted,  as  if 
in  expectation  of  being  laid  in  her  mother's  arms,  or  when  already 
in  her  mother's  lap,  of  being  raised  to  the  breast.  On  the 
49th  day,  I  saw  her  open  her  mouth  for  the  nipple  when  her 
mother  chanced  to  lift  her  into  the  nursing  position,  though 
it  was  not  nursing  time,  she  was  not  hungry,  and  the  dress  was 
not  open,  and  up  to  the  tenth  week,  she  would  as  readily  stop  her 
cries  when  lifted  to  any  one  else's  arm  as  her  mother's.  The  early 
association  between  sucking  movements  and  hunger  (45th  day)  has 
been  spoken  of  just  above.  Another  early  association  connected 
with  hunger,  approaching  nearer  to  real  memory,  was  due  to  the 
mother's  habit  of  washing  out  the  baby's  mouth  with  a  soft  cloth 
before  nursing  her;  on  the  47th  day  the  baby  stopped  fretting  when 
this  was  done,  just  as  when  lifted  in  the  arms  to  be  nursed;  and 
afterward  showed  in  other  ways  plainly  that  she  associated  the 
ceremony  with  the  coming  satisfaction  of  hunger. 

The  playing,  or  experimenting,  with  the  tongue  and  lips  that  ap- 
peared in  the  seventh  week,  I  have  referred  to  interest  in  touch  sensa- 
tions (p.  1 36,  above).  So  too  with  the  tendency  to  put  everything 
to  the  mouth,  to  lick,  suck,  and  bite,  so  conspicuous  in  the  first 
year:  I  refer  it  more  to  touch  interest,  and  to  ancestral  habit  now 
become  meaningless,  than  other  observers  have  done. l  Neverthe- 
less, association  with  the  pleasure  of  food  in  the  baby's  individual 
experience  may  have  its  influence,  in  both  cases.  This  tendency 
appeared  in  my  niece  in  the   tenth   week,    with  putting   the  fists 

1  "Probably  we  have  here  a  case  of  primitive  logical  inference:  up  to  this 
time  sucking  and  tasting  were  the  most  important  strong,  agreeable  sensations 
the  young  being  has  known;  when,  therefore,  he  has  a  new  agreeable  sensation 
(e.  g.,  of  a  bright  color,  a  round,  smooth  body,  a  soft  surface),  it  is  brought 
into  association  with  the  lips  and  tongue,  through  which  the  pleasurable  feeling 
at  taking  in  the  sweet  milk  was  received. "  Preyer,  The  Senses  and  the  Will, 
p.  249. 


214  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

to  the  mouth  constantly,  sucking  and  mumbling  whatever  fraction 
of  them  she  could  get  inside,  and  putting  them  back  when  taken 
away;  of  its  farther  development  I  shall  speak  under  a  later  head. 
Opening  the  mouth  as  an  expression  of  pleasure  (twelfth  week), 
may  have  been  a  reminiscence  of  the  breast. 

In  spite  of  these  indications  of  the  importance  of  the  sensation, 
and  the  imperativeness  of  the  demand  for  food  sometimes  in  the 
first  and  second  months,  I  could  not  see  that  hunger,  even  in  these 
earliest  months,  was  as  important  in  consciousness,  compared  with 
other  sensations,  as  I  looked  to  find  it.  In  the  ninth  and  tenth 
weeks  the  desire  to  be  lifted  to  a  sitting  position  was  expressed  by 
just  such  fretting  at  first  and  imperative  wailing  afterwards  as  in  the 
case  of  hunger;  and  there  was  on  the  whole  less  crying  over  hunger 
than  over  being  laid  down  flat  when  she  wished  to  be  erect.  In  its 
first  stages,  hunger  could  be  driven  from  consciousness  for  many 
minutes  by  other  sensations.  During  the  second  month  and  most 
of  the  third,  the  sound  of  the  piano  would  stop  the  fretting  for  five 
or  ten  minutes,  —  rarely  the  outright  crying  after  hunger  had  become 
imperative,  but  I  note  one  instance  (45th  day)  when  the  fretting  had 
almost  reached  the  wailing  point  before  the  baby  was  carried  to  the 
piano,  but  as  soon  as  the  notes  began  she  became  quiet,  and  lay 
perfectly  content  and  attentive  some  five  minutes,  then  began  to 
move,  purse  her  lips,  and  at  last  to  fret  a  little;  we  did  not  prolong 
the  experiment  to  see  how  soon  the  actual  point  of  wailing  would 
be  reached  again,  as  the  time  for  nursing  had  now  arrived.  Watch- 
ing the  piano  keys  while  some  one  played,  together  with  listening, 
diverted  her  completely  from  hunger  for  ten  minutes  or  more  on 
the  57th  day.  It  was  easier  to  divert  her  from  hunger  (I  note  in  the 
tenth  week)  than  from  the  discomfort  of  being  dressed.  On  the 
46th  day,  though  she  had  been  fretting  with  hunger  for  some 
minutes,  when  at  last  I  laid  her  in  her  mother's  arms  she  chanced 
to  catch  sight  of  some  white  clothes  hanging  in  the  sunshine  out- 
side the  window,  and  her  attention  was  absorbed  in  the  sight  for 
some  seconds.  At  another  time  (56th  day)  she  caught  sight  of  a 
red  ribbon  at  her  mother's  neck,  stopped  nursing,  and  would  not  go 
on  till  the  interesting  object  was  put  out  of  sight.  In  the  fourth 
month  she  often  stopped  nursing  to  throw  back  her  head,  look  at 
her  mother's  face,  and  smile.     The   I22d  day  she  repeatedly  lifted 


Shimn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  215 

her  head  to  look  at  me  if  I  spoke  while  she  was  nursing.  The  1 33d 
day,  after  nursing  a  little,  she  sat  up  and  looked  at  me  with  a  broad 
smile  of  satisfaction,  then  began  to  bend  herself  back  as  if  in  renewal 
of  the  experiment  in  touch  and  muscular  sensation  she  had  been 
engaged  in  (see  p.  198,  above),  caught  sight  of  a  brass  caster  on  a 
chair,  and  remained  so  fixedly  gazing  at  it  that  she  could  not  be 
induced  to  resume  nursing  till  she  had  lost  sight  of  it  (see  Sight, 
p.  81).  In  the  sixth  month,  after  showing  some  eagerness  and  im- 
patience when  about  to  be  nursed,  as  mentioned  above,  diving  at 
the  breast  as  soon  as  it  was  uncovered  and  getting  the  nipple  into 
her  mouth  herself,  she  would  nurse  eagerly  for  but  a  minute  or  two, 
then  very  leisurely,  stopping  at  intervals  to  sit  up  and  play,  return- 
ing from  time  to  time  to  the  breast  for  a  little  more.  The  157th 
day,  e.  g.,  I  came  into  the  room  while  she  was  nursing :  she  looked 
up  at  my  entrance  and  began  to  laugh,  sat  up  and  put  out  her 
hands,  and  when  I  knelt  down  beside  her,  played  with  my  head 
awhile,  then  sat  looking  gravely  and  attentively  at  me  while  I  talked 
to  her  mother,  breaking  into  smiles  when  I  looked  at  her  or  spoke 
to  her;  presently  she  began  to  reach  about  for  something  to  get 
hold  of;  she  could  not  be  persuaded  to  resume  nursing  for  some 
time.  Soon  other  occasions:  she  was  almost  always  ready,  after 
satisfying  the  first  hunger,  to  desert  the  breast  for  a  frolic,  or  to 
listen,  look  about,  and  play;  I  find  note  of  it  up  to  the  last  week  of 
the  eighth  month.  See  Sight,  p.  24,  for  her  diversion  from  nursing 
by  interest  in  a  knot  of  cords  on  an  ottoman,  —  a  striking  instance, 
because  of  her  remembering  the  knot  all  the  time  she  was  nursing, 
and  sitting  up  again  from  time  to  time  to  look  for  it. 

Sleep  was  not  easily  broken  by  hunger.  Six  hours  was  a 
common  period  of  sleep  in  the  first  and  second  months,  and  seven 
hours  not  infrequent  in  the  second  month ;  on  the  58th  day  she 
slept  eight  hours  continuously,  and  on  the  63d  day,  nine  and  one- 
fourth  hours,  —  two  hours  being  all  this  time  the  regular  interval 
between  meals,  and  sometimes  too  long  an  interval  for  her  desire 
when  awake.  Though  sleep  no  doubt  postponed  greatly  the  con- 
ditions of  hunger,  these  must  have  arrived  and  reached  considerable 
intensity  before  the  end  of  these  longest  periods,  yet  without  break- 
ing her  sleep.  Her  behavior  immediately  on  awaking  showed 
hunger   sharply  present:  in  the  case  of  the  eight-hour  sleep,  she 


216  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

waked  with  a  frantic  cry  of  hunger,  that  roused  a  sound  sleeper  in 
the  room  above ;  after  the  nine  and  a  quarter  hour  sleep,  she  did 
not  cry,  but  fell  to  sucking  her  fist  vigorously  as  soon  as  she  was 
awake.  In  one  case  (37th  day)  she  dropped  asleep  on  returning 
from  a  drive,  before  she  could  be  put  to  the  breast,  just  at  the  close 
of  the  two-hour  interval,  when  hunger  must  have  been  already 
present  or  close  at  hand,  and  slept  four  hours;  and  even  then 
showed  no  sign  of  waking,  so  her  mother  waked  her  to  nurse. 
In  the  eighth  and  ninth  months  she  repeatedly  slept  through  the 
midnight  nursing  time,  thus  dropping  out  one  of  her  daily  meals. 

Nor  did  I  find  the  satisfaction  of  hunger  the  great,  dominant 
pleasure  of  the  early  months,  as  other  observers  have  done.  She 
nursed  from  the  first  with  energy,  and  with  an  expression  of  great 
content,  (eyes  closed,  as  I  have  noted  above,  Sight,  p.  12;  not 
open,  as  Preyer  records);  in  the  second  month,  she  uttered  little 
grunts  as  she  sucked,  —  probably  merely  from  losing  breath,  but 
they  certainly  had  a  sound  of  high  satisfaction,  and  similar  sounds 
were  made  when  she  was  lying  in  great  general  contentment  on 
the  lounge.  Still,  from  the  first,  there  was  just  as  marked  an 
appearance  of  pleasure  in  the  bath,  and  after  the  middle  of  the 
second  month,  demonstrations  of  much  higher  pleasure  in  sights, 
such  as  faces,  a  spot  of  sunlight,  strips  of  color;  these  she  greeted 
with  movements  of  hands  and  feet,  while  she  panted  in  short, 
audible  breaths,  —  or  in  the  third  month,  with  smiles  and  murmurs. 
Once  (47th  day)  she  showed  this  high  enjoyment,  with  movements 
and  panting,  from  a  general  sense  of  freedom  and  well-being,  as 
she  lay  naked  after  her  bath;  but  never  in  connection  with  food. 
So  again  in  the  fifth  month  I  note  that  her  smiles,  crows,  gurgles, 
and  movements  of  hands  and  feet  are  for  pleasure  chiefly  in  faces 
and  voices,  and  secondly  in  general  well-being,  but  not  in  eating. 
Her  first  special  interest  in  a  person  (in  the  fifth  month)  was  not 
in  her  mother,  the  source  of  supplies,  but  in  her  grandfather,  who 
played  with  her  most  to  her  liking. 

She  never  took  more  food  than  she  wanted,  and  regurgitation 
of  the  milk  scarcely  happened  half  a  dozen  times  during  the  whole 
period  of  nursing.  From  the  fourth  month  she  would  stop  de- 
cisively when  she  had  had  enough,  thrusting  out  the  nipple  and 
turning  her  head  away. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  21  J 

Though  hunger  and  its  satisfaction  played  a  smaller  part  in 
the  baby's  direct  consciousness  than  I  had  expected,  its  effect  on 
her  general  condition  and  mood  was  controlling.  With  the 
beginning  of  the  second  month  it  began  to  be  noticeable  that 
after  being  fed,  if  she  was  at  the  same  time  warm,  dry,  wide  awake, 
and  in  entire  health,  she  was  not  only  in  the  highest  good  temper, 
but  at  the  best  in  activity  and  mental  alertness;  in  at  least  one 
case  (47th  day),  I  noticed  at  such  a  time  a  very  notable  advance 
step  in  development  (see  Grasping,  below). 

The  137th  and  138th  days  she  wanted  constantly  to  be  amused, 
making  sounds  of  dissatisfaction  and  desire.  When  diverted  with 
a  plaything  she  would  cease  the  whimper  and  utter  happy  gurgles ; 
but  by  the  afternoon  of  the  138th  day  she  grew  more  and  more 
restless,  and  kept  up  her  complaining  cry  incessantly  except  when 
walked  with.  As  the  child  seemed  to  be  in  perfect  health  and 
abundantly  fed,  it  was  hard  to  tell  what  was  the  trouble,  but  the 
grandmother  said  that  she  seemed  like  a  baby  in  need  of  nourish- 
ment, and  that  though  the  mother's  milk  was  apparently  quite  suf- 
ficient, it  might  have  been  affected  in  quality  by  a  toothache  from 
which  she  had  been  suffering  for  days,  and  advised  a  little  supple- 
mentary nourishment;  so  the  baby  was  at  once  given  four  table- 
spoonfuls  of  cream  and  hot  water.  The  effect  was  quite  magical, 
and  I  was  called  downstairs  about  half  a  minute  after  the  dose  had 
been  administered,  to  see  a  transformed  baby  kicking  and  smiling 
on  the  rug  in  perfect  content.  For  some  days  one  daily  meal  of 
this  diluted  cream,  supplementing  the  mother's  milk,  kept  her  well- 
fed  and  content. 

Apparently  in  this  case  the  discomfort  had  not  been  due  to  any 
definite  sensation  of  hunger,  of  which  no  signs  had  been  shown, 
but  to  a  general  organic  discomfort  from  defective  nutrition.  It 
could  not  have  been  due  to  any  toxic  condition  of  the  mother's 
milk,  but  only  to  a  slight  impoverishment  in  its  quality,  since  the 
cream  was  substituted  for  the  natural  milk  only  once  a  day.  It  was 
noticeable  that  the  presence  of  food  in  the  stomach  instantly  re- 
lieved the  discomfort,  apparently  before  the  effect  through  the  cir- 
culation could  have  been  felt;  but  it  is  common  enough  with  every 
one  to  have  food  instantly  relieve  faintness,  depression,  and  other 
apparently  remote  effects  of  insufficient  nourishment. 


2  1 8  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

On  the  171st  day  occurred  another  instance  of  instantaneous 
relief  from  great  general  discomfort :  the  child  had  been  for  some 
hours  sleepy,  tired,  hungry,  frightened  by  a  stranger,  and  separated 
from  her  mother,  whom  she  had  for  the  first  time  missed  and  cried 
for;  but  when  at  last  she  had  been  fed,  she  rolled  over  on  the  bed 
from  her  mother's  breast,  looked  up  and  laughed  in  my  face,  and 
was  ready  to  come  to  me  and  play,  though  she  had  been  crying  in 
my  arms  just  before,  afraid  of  being  separated  from  her  mother. 

The  foregoing  notes  concern  entirely  the  period  of  nursing. 
Between  the  twenty-third  and  forty-sixth  weeks  (see  detailed  ac- 
count of  diet  at  the  end  of  this  chapter)  this  period  overlapped  that 
of  general  food,  as  the  mother's  milk  failed  and  was  gradually 
replaced  by  other  nourishment.  There  was  thus  at  no  time  any 
definite  weaning,  and  other  habits  of  food  were  entirely  established 
by  the  time  nursing  was  given  up;  the  baby  was  hardly  aware  of 
the  transition.  Nevertheless,  in  the  first  experiences  of  artificial 
feeding,  the  discomfort  of  taking  milk  from  a  spoon  as  compared 
with  nursing  at  the  breast  was  sometimes  obstinately  resented,  and 
now  and  then  it  was  necessary,  in  order  to  get  it  coaxed  down  at 
all,  to  divert  the  baby's  attention  with  a  little  bell  while  the  spoon 
was  placed  to  her  lips;  she  would  then  swallow  the  milk  absently, 
without  noticing  and  protesting  against  the  spoon.  A  little  later 
(twenty-fourth  week)  she  discovered  that  she  could  scatter  the  milk 
about  by  a  sputter  with  her  lips  just  as  the  spoon  reached  them, 
and  preferred  this  to  swallowing  it,  so  a  bottle  was  substituted,  with 
which  she  tried  in  vain  to  play  the  same  trick.  In  the  eighth 
month  a  cup  succeeded  the  bottle;  this  too  was  disliked,  and  she 
had  to  be  coaxed  and  urged  through  the  daily  meal  that  was  taken 
from  it.  To  be  urged  to  take  food  from  either  cup  or  bottle  when 
she  was  sleepy  was  a  serious  grievance,  over  which  she  sometimes 
cried  hard. 

During  the  whole  period  of  transition  there  was  much  fluctuation 
in  the  baby's  appetite,  and  as  her  food  became  more  varied,  it  was 
impossible  to  tell  how  much  of  her  behavior  toward  it  was  due  to 
taste,  and  how  much  to  organic  sensation.  Hunger  was  scarcely 
ever  shown:  once  (266th  day)  the  baby  cried  for  the  breast  when 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  219 

she  was  put  off  after  the  time  had  come  and  she  had  been  taken 
into  her  mother's  lap  to  be  nursed;  I  mention  with  this  note  that 
it  is  rare  for  her  to  appear  thus  eager  and  hungry.  During  a  rail- 
road trip  in  the  forty-third  week,  when  she  was  hungry  she  would 
point  to  the  satchel  in  which  crackers  and  a  bottle  of  milk  were 
kept,  and  once,  having  eaten  all  the  cracker  in  her  hand,  she 
pointed  to  the  floor  with  an  urgent  sound,  for  the  crumbs.  In  the 
same  week  she  expressed  desire  for  food  by  begging  sounds,  rising 
once  to  a  clamor  of  impatience.  A  few  days  later  (304th  day)  when 
her  mush  appeared  at  the  breakfast  table,  she  pointed  to  it  with 
laughter  and  clamor  of  eagerness.  Desire  for  food,  and  pleasure  in 
eating  it,  were  not  infrequent,  but  it  was  not  usually  as  clearly  due 
to  appetite  as  it  seemed  to  be  in  these  cases,  but  rather  to  taste, 
some  favorite  food  being  the  object  of  desire.  New  foods  were 
sometimes  accepted  with  eagerness,  but  after  a  few  weeks  ceased  to 
excite  pleasure  (see  above,  Taste,  p.  163).  The  most  consistent 
demonstrations  of  desire  and  pleasure  were  for  fruit-juice  (Taste, 
p.  165),  which  could  hardly  have  had  much  to  do  with  the  satisfac- 
tion of  hunger,  yet  may  have  met  some  organic  need.  In  the 
fortieth  week,  she  would  refuse  to  take  her  milk  in  her  high-chair 
at  the  table,  and  beg  to  be  put  down  on  the  floor,  where  she  was 
willing  to  take  it  in  small  doses,  interspersed  with  play;  she  would 
from  time  to  time  creep  to  her  mother's  side,  pull  herself  to  her  feet, 
and  hold  up  her  head  and  open  her  mouth  for  a  drink  of  milk,  then 
creep  away  again.  Lack  of  appetite  and  indifference  to  food  are 
often  mentioned  in  my  notes,  usually  when  there  was  no  ill  health 
to  account  for  it.  In  the  tenth  and  eleventh  months  especially,  as 
the  proportion  of  food  that  could  be  had  from  the  breast  became 
inconsiderable,  it  was  difficult  to  get  the  baby  to  take  enough  food 
for  proper  nourishment,  and  she  grew  perceptibly  thinner;  she  had 
been  during  the  first  half-year  a  remarkably  plump  baby,  and  it  was 
not  till  the  forty-second  week  that  the  deep  crease  about  the  wrist 
disappeared.  Under  the  circumstances,  with  the  advice  of  the 
family  doctor,  she  was  given  a  more  solid  and  varied  diet  than  is 
usually  allowed  at  that  age  (see  below,  p.  231);  immediately  on  its 
adoption  her  appetite  revived,  and  it  seemed  to  agree  well  with  her  ; 
her  weight  also,  which  had  decreased  in  the  tenth  month,  returned 


220  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i 

to  its  former  rate  of  increase.1  Most  of  the  signs  of  hunger  and 
genuine  eagerness  for  food  mentioned  above  were  at  this  time.  By 
the  forty-fifth  week,  however,  the  indifference  to  food  is  again  noted, 
and  she  wished  to  diversify  the  uninteresting  process  of  eating  in 
all  sorts  of  ways.  For  example,  the  3 1 5th  day,  after  taking  a  little 
food  at  lunch,  she  began  to  intimate  by  complaining  sounds  and 
look  and  motion  toward  the  floor,  her  desire  to  be  set  down;  after 
taking  a  few  spoonfuls  standing  on  the  floor,  she  crept  over  to  her 
grandmother  and  asked  her  for  more ;  then  begged  with  outstretched 
arms  to  come  to  me,  accepted  a  spoonful  or  two  from  me,  then 
wished  to  get  down  to  the  floor  again.  In  the  afternoon  she  greeted 
the  appearance  of  her  mush  with  laughter  and  motions  of  joy,  and 
scrambled  from  my  lap  to  her  mother's  to  get  it,  but  soon  tired  of 
it,  and  the  rest  had  to  be  coaxed  down.  The  324th  day  she  began 
to  take  her  food  readily  enough,  sitting  on  her  mother's  lap,  but 
soon  stopped  eating  to  point  at  her  uncle's  hat  with  a  sound  of 
desire,  and  intimate  a  wish  that  I  should  put  it  on ;  next  she  wished 
to  come  from  her  mother's  lap  to  mine  and  be  fed  there;  then 
lay  back  on  my  arm,  and  let  her  mother  feed  her  so  for  a  few 
spoonfuls;  soon  tiring,  began  to  scramble  about  and  play  with  the 
hat,  and  coax  us  successively  to  put  it  on,  refusing  to  take  any 
farther  interest  in  food. 

The  last  daily  nursing  (at  midnight)  was  given  up  in  the  forty- 
sixth  week,  a  glass  of  milk  being  substituted  for  it  after  a  few  days. 
The  general  conditions  of  appetite  continued  the  same  as  during  the 
last  two  months  before  weaning  was  complete:  I  constantly  note 
indifference  to  food,  and  difficulty  in  finding  things  that  she  will  eat. 

1  In  an  interesting  record  by  Mrs.  Edith  Elmer  Wood  (B.  L.,  Smith  College, 
'90),  which  has  been  kindly  put  into  my  hands,  I  find  the  note  that  her  boy  was 
weaned  near  the  end  of  the  12th  month.  He  fought  against  the  change  the 
first  day,  was  plaintive  for  several  days,  then  accepted  the  situation  and  ate 
with  avidity,  though  up  to  the  15th  month  he  made  frequent  efforts  to  nurse. 
He  refused  a  nursing  bottle  from  the  first,  and  drank  from  a  cup.  He  was 
given  milk,  oatmeal,  potatoes,  Imperial  Granum,  beef  juice,  chicken  broth,  and 
toast, —  a  dietary  not  very  different  from  that  of  my  niece  at  the  same  age.  It 
appeared  to  agree  with  him,  and  he  improved  at  once  in  sleeping,  waking  only 
once  and  taking  only  one  meal  between  5:30  p.  M.  and  8  A.  M.  Six  months 
later  his  mother  records  that  he  has  always  been  in  perfect  physical  condition, 
and  has  never  had  a  really  sick  day  in  his  life.  Mrs.  Wood's  table  of  weights, 
however,  indicates  a  slightly  checked  growth  at  the  time  of  weaning. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  22  r 

She  would  not  eat  more  than  three  times  a  day,  (once  only  I  note 
that  she  gets  a  fraction  of  a  fourth  meal  at  the  family  dinner,  some- 
what later  than  her  own);  and  even  when  she  liked  some  offered 
food  pretty  well,  she  had  to  be  followed  about  with  it  and  coaxed  to 
eat.  She  would  sit  in  her  high-chair  and  take  a  little,  then  beg  to 
get  down;  creep  under  the  table  and  find  her  grandmother,  get  into 
her  lap  and  take  a  little  more,  then  slip  down  again  and  creep  about 
the  floor,  take  a  little  more  standing  by  her  mother,  etc.  "Prefers 
playing  about  to  eating,"  I  note  in  the  forty-eighth  week.  Occa- 
sionally pleasure  in  some  food,  or  comparatively  good  appetite,  is 
noted,  but  the  rule  is  indifference  to  food.  Since  this  readiness  to 
desert  food  for  play,  or  to  intersperse  eating  with  attention  to  other 
things,  was  equally  marked  in  the  nursing  period,  (p.  215,  above)  it 
had  nothing  to  do  with  the  change  of  food.1 

It  will  be  remarked  at  once  that  hunger  is  a  sensation  which 
has  little  chance  to  make  itself  felt  in  a  well  cared  for  child,  whose 
needs  are  always  anticipated :  and  it  is  true  that  when  this  baby's 
lunch  was  once  late  for  an  hour,  (at  just  eleven  months  old,)  making 
about  5  )/2  hours' interval,  there  was  a  perceptible  increase  of  appetite; 
and  again  in  the  same  week,  when  she  had  slept  three  hours  later 
than  usual,  an  hour  or  two  past  her  breakfast-time,  her  first  act  on 
waking  was  to  climb  to  her  feet  in  the  crib  and  reach  for  the  cup 
that  held  the  remains  of  her  midnight  lunch.  But  I  should  certainly 
have  expected  that  an  active  and  healthy  child,  even  though  regu- 
larly fed,  would  find  the  interval  between  three  meals  a  day  long 
enough  for  a  considerable  development  of  appetite. 

1  Mrs.  Beatty's  record  gives  an  instance  of  the  suspension  of  interest  in  food 
by  a  more  mental  interest,  in  the  eleventh  month.  She  had  brought  her  baby 
in  from  a  ride,  and  gave  him  to  the  nurse  to  hold  while  she  prepared  his 
supper ;  and  tht  nurse,  having  occasion  to  go  to  the  basement,  where  the  kittens 
lived,  carried  him  with  her,  and  came  back  without  stopping  to  show  him  the 
kittens.  The  baby  began  to  cry,  and  the  nurse,  guessing  the  trouble,  asked  if 
it  was  the  kittens  he  wanted ;  he  answered  by  demonstrations  of  desire,  but  his 
mother  now  put  him  in  his  high-chair,  and  offered  his  food.  The  baby  refused  to 
touch  a  mouthful,  turning  from  it  and  holding  out  his  hands  to  the  nurse  and 
crying,  till  she  took  him  to  see  the  kittens ;  after  which  he  ate  his  supper 
contentedly. 

Mrs.  Sharpe's  boy  at  one  year,  though  he  habitually  preferred  his  mother, 
would  leave  her  readily  for  the  maid  at  the  suggestion  of  food. 


2  22  University  of  California.  [vol.  i. 

With  the  second  year  a  considerable  increase  of  interest  in  food 
was  visible.  I  find  but  one  reference  to  downright  hunger  ("  Waked 
from  a  nap  very  hungry"),  in  the  seventy-ninth  week;  but  the  child 
often  asked  for  something  to  eat  between  meal-times,  and  was  usually 
given  something,  fruit,  cracker,  or  cooky,  so  that  there  was  little 
opportunity  for  hunger.  Her  appetite  was  good  as  a  rule,  and  her 
food  enjoyed,  and  she  sometimes  showed  eagerness  for  it.  These 
expressions  of  eagerness,  however,  were  almost  always  for  some 
favorite  food,  so  that  it  was  impossible  to  say  how  far  the  desire  of 
agreeable  taste  caused  them,  and  how  far  appetite.  Thus  in  the 
fifty-third  week  she  clamored  for  her  sweet  potato  all  the  time  it 
was  being  prepared;  in  the  fifty-sixth,  and  at  intervals  later  during 
the  year,  she  would  squeal  and  shout  for  sundry  desired  articles  of 
food,  and  sometimes  ask  for  more  when  her  portion  was  eaten. 
Once  in  the  seventy-seventh  week  I  showed  her  some  small  new 
potatoes  cooking,  and  told  her  they  were  for  her;  she  appropriated 
them  as  soon  as  they  appeared  at  table,  with  a  jubilant  yell  of, 
"Taty!"  and  ate  them  all  (four)  with  zeal.  See  also  instances  of 
pleasure  in  food  under  Taste,  above;  especially  pp.  168,  169. 

Though  eating  counted  for  more  than  in  the  first  year  among 
her  pleasures  and  desires,  doubtless  because  the  gratification  of  taste 
was  now  added  to  the  satisfaction  of  appetite,  yet  even  with  this  re- 
enforcement,  it  is  quite  striking  to  see  how  much  larger  a  part  see- 
ing, hearing,  and  doing  played  than  eating.  In  the  fifteenth  month 
I  once  note  that  interest  in  food  suspends  all  other  interests;  and 
once  in  the  twentieth  month  that  fruit,  sweets,  and  meat  rank 
with  going  outdoors  as  the  objects  of  prime  desire  ;  once  also,  in  the 
thirteenth  month,  that  favorite  foods  are  next  after  going  outdoors 
the  chief  object  of  desire:  but  with  these  exceptions  food  is  always 
mentioned  in  a  subordinate  place  among  the  regular  objects  of 
desire,  after  such  things  as  pictures,  learning  words,  exploring  up- 
stairs, jouncing  on  mattress  springs,  dolls,  playing  with  books,  etc. 
Among  occasional  pleasures  also,  a  host  are  recorded  that  gave  live- 
lier joy  than  food:  such  as  going  out  to  see  the  moon,  which  caused 
a  "passion  of  joy"  (thirteenth  month);  using  a  fork,  in  the  fifteenth 
month;  carrying  a  muff,  in  the  same  month.  Other  interests  still 
easily  diverted  her  from  food,  and  she  was  almost  without  exception 
reluctant  to  leave  play,  and  especially  outdoor  play,  for  her  meals* 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  223 

See  Taste,  p.  169,  for  several  instances;  some  others  may  be  men- 
tioned:—  At  seventeen  months,  brought  indoors  to  dinner,  she 
cried  to  go  out  again ;  in  the  nineteenth  month,  called  to  go  down- 
stairs to  lunch  while  playing  with  my  books,  she  went  readily,  say- 
ing, "See  grandma!"  but  once  there,  begged  to  go  back,  and 
consented  with  very  ill  grace  to  eat  her  lunch;  in  the  twentieth 
month  she  said,  "No,"  plumply  when  told  to  leave  her  play  with 
my  books  and  go  to  dinner,  and  when  carried  off,  begged  hard  to 
go  back,  with  dinner  before  her  eyes;  later  in  the  same  month,  at  a 
restaurant,  she  was  excited  and  eager  over  the  novel  surroundings, 
slipping  down  from  her  chair  and  trying  to  run  off  and  explore 
everything,  crying  out  over  things  seen,  with  slight  interest  in  lunch. 
In  the  last  week  of  the  twenty-first  month,  she  utterly  refused  to  go 
indoors  on  hearing  the  dinner-bell.  "Did  not  Ruth  want  lunch?  " 
"  No."  "  Not  even  if  there  were  raspberries  ?  "  "  No."  In  the  last 
week  of  the  twenty-second  month,  she  so  resented  being  brought 
in  from  outdoors  that  she  did  not  want  dinner,  and  when  set  in  her 
high-chair  appealed,  "Papa  take  Tootyboo  down?"  "Aunty  take 
Tootyboo  down  ? "  and  when  refused,  began  to  cry,  then  essayed, 
"  Want  see  dear  grandma ! "  and  when  this  failed,  broke  out  into  her 
rare  wail,  with  tears.  She  was  reconciled,  however,  when  food  came. 
In  one  case  (eighty-fifth  week),  she  was  diverted  from  the  desire  to 
go  outdoors  by  dinner,  but  it  was  the  only  time  I  have  recorded,  and 
she  was  influenced  by  the  promise  that  she  should  go  out  after  dinner, 
—  a  promise  that  she  claimed  the  instant  she  was  set  down  from  the 
table.  I  have  also  a  single  note  (twenty-first  month)  of  her  going 
cheerfully  into  the  house  when  lunch  was  announced,  and  the  note 
expressly  comments  on  it  as  rare.  For  one  or  two  instances,  on 
the  other  hand,  of  the  subordination  of  other  interests  by  the  idea  of 
food,  see  Taste,  p.  169. 

Desire  for  food,  up  to  the  sixteenth  month,  was  regularly  ex- 
pressed by  the  same  asking  sound  that  indicated  other  desires,  often 
with  pointing;  occasional  outcries  of  eagerness  are  mentioned 
above.  During  the  fifteenth  month,  smacking  the  lips  was  again, 
as  early  in  the  first  year,  a  frequent  sign  of  desire  for  food. 

I  have  but  a  single  note  of  what  might  be  called  a  sensation  of 


224  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

pleasure  in  satiety:  in  the  eighteenth  month,  after  a  good  dinner, 
(535th  day)  she  "lolled  back  in  her  chair,  in  a  condition  of  ridiculous 
felicity,  grunting  with  contented  repletion."  After  this  she  became 
active,  and  ran  about  in  great  spirits;  and  I  note  again  at  the  begin- 
ning of  the  twentieth  month  that  "after  dinner  she  was  in  boiling- 
over  spirits;"  but  this  happy  activity  was  so  common  that  I  was  not 
able  to  establish  any  regular  connection  between  it  and  a  preceding 
meal. 

In  the  third  year,  although  her  appetite  seems  now  to  have  been 
almost  uniformly  good,  and  there  was  no  trouble  in  having  her  take 
food  enough,  eating  played  a  still  smaller  part  *in  her  desires  and 
pleasures  than  in  the  second  year,  and  is  rarely  mentioned  at  all  in 
my  record  of  them.  Fruit  and  sweets  were  strongly  desired  dur- 
ing the  whole  year,  and  meat  at  times ;  but  these  were  desires  mainly 
referable  to  taste,  I  thought.  Once  in  the  twenty-sixth  month  she 
was  so  interested  in  what  she  regarded  as  "writing  a  letter  to  Cousin 
Teddy"  that  she  could  not  make  up  her  mind  to  leave  it,  till  dinner 
was  almost  over;  and  as  she  was  making  a  careful  and  interesting 
piece  of  imitative  scribbling,  she  was  allowed  to  finish  it,  and  sat  by 
the  table  where  others  were  eating  dinner,  diligently  hatching 
little  marks  across  a  sheet  of  paper,  without  the  least  interest  in  the 
food  before  her;  and  after  the  "letter"  was  done,  she  insisted  on 
going  and  putting  away  her  pencil  before  she  would  climb  into  her 
high-chair  for  dinner. 

One  or  two  instances  of  real  hunger  were  noted  in  this  year, 
however.  In  one  case  there  had  been  both  an  unusual  exhaustion 
of  energy  and  a  postponement  of  the  hour  of  eating.  This  was  on 
the  first  day  of  the  year.  She  had  taken  the  thirty-mile  journey 
to  the  city,  and  had  experienced  much  pleasurable  excitement  in 
getting  a  new  head  put  on  her  doll  at  a  toy-shop ;  and  had  then 
posed  at  a  slow  photographer's,  and  tried  to  please  with  a  dili- 
gence that  was  fairly  pathetic  for  some  twenty  minutes,  —  letting 
him  put  the  head-rest  behind  her  head,  holding  up  or  lowering  her 
chin  when  told,  looking  in  the  direction  he  charged  her  to,  and 
holding  back  her  eyes  from  wandering  to  her  mother  and  me,  with- 
out a  single  murmur  or  relaxation  of  effort;  not  mere  passive  obe- 
dience, for  the  effort  required  so  carefully  to  conform  for  so  long, 


Shinn.]  77;,?  Development  of  a    Child.  225 

was  very  great  for  so  young  a  child,  and  required  exertion  of  her 
own  will.  On  a  second  trial  she  evidently  tried  again  to  obey 
directions,  but  being  tired  and  hungry,  had  not  the  patience  or  had 
not  the  nervous  control  for  keeping  a  pose,  and  would  try  a  little, 
then  turn  and  drop  to  the  floor,  saying  Ruth  would  sit  down.  She 
hailed  the  idea  of  going  thence  and  especially  of  lunch;  and  (being 
especially  on  her  good  behaviorthat  day)asked  politely  but  wistfully 
several  times  before  we  reached  the  restaurant,  "Mamma,  may  Ruth 
have  lunch?"  "Aunty,  may  Ruth  have  something  to  eat?"  When 
we  reached  the  restaurant,  and  she  found  herself  seated  before  a  bare 
table  where  she  had  expected  food,  she  quivered  into  tears,  but 
pulled  herself  together  in  the  most  reasonable  way  when  assured 
that  food  was  coming.  On  hearing  a  cup  of  milk  ordered,  she 
added  very  anxiously,  "And  something!"  and  after  repeating  this 
without  an  answer,  broke  down  again,  and  again  stopped  crying  as 
soon  as  she  was  told  she  should  have  a  proper  lunch.  After  lunch, 
though  her  joyous  spirit  of  the  morning  was  toned  by  a  little  sleepi- 
ness, she  seemed  entirely  recovered  from  any  nervous  fatigue,  and  it 
may  have  been  only  hunger  that  had  caused  her  unusual  tears  be- 
fore lunch. 

Once  in  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  cried  clamorously  when 
told  that  she  must  not  have  anything  to  eat  till  dinner;  and  at 
another  time,  wished  to  come  back  after  leaving  the  table,  saying, 
"I  did  n't  finish  my  breakfast." 

Of  interest  and  attention  excited  in  connection  with  hunger  and 
eating,  —  her  own  consciousness  of  the  sensation  and  act,  —  I 
noticed  no  evidence  in  the  first  year,  unless  acquiring  the  names  of 
eatables  could  come  under  this  head:  at  the  beginning  of  the  elev- 
enth month,  out  of  84  words  whose  meaning  was  understood,  milk 
and  cracker  were  the  only  food  names;  see  also  Taste,  p.  173. 

Late  in  the  sixteenth  month  (480th  day)  she  pointed  to  the  fish- 
wagon,  exclaiming,  "Mea'!"  —  "No,  fish,"  some  one  explained:  "the 
man  is  giving  fish  to  Gan."  —  "Mea'?"  she  repeated  doubtfully  sev- 
eral times;  then,  receiving  the  same  answer,  "Fish,"  each  time, 
she  changed  to  "Fi'l"  and  after  repeating  this  several  times,  after  her 
fashion,  added,  "Da'  ?"  —  "Yes,  to  Gan."— "Ea'?"— "Yes,  for  us  tc 
eat"    She  pointed  to  her  plate:  "Ea'?"  — "Yes,  that  is  for  Ruth  tc 


226  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

eat,  too."  —  "F6' !  "  —  "Yes,  with  her  fork."  After  this,  curiosity 
about  eating  and  edibles  seemed  to  increase.  She  would  ask  if  she 
might  put  things  into  her  mouth  by  putting  them  toward  it,  with 
"Ea'?"  When  told  of  a  calla,  "No,  indeed,  it  is  very  bad!"  she 
repeated,  "Ba' ,"  many  times.  She  found  one  of  the  dog's  old 
bones  (484th  day),  and  asked,  "Ea'?"  and  carefully  breaking  off  a 
bit  of  the  dirt  that  crusted  it,  called  it  "mea' ,"  and  offered  it  to  the 
dog.  Told  that  the  meat  was  all  gone  and  the  bone  no  good,  she 
repeated,  "Goo'  —  no,  no  !  "  shaking  her  head  and  trotting  away.  In 
the  seventeenth  and  eighteenth  months  she  would  offer  the  dog  any 
article  that  came  handy,  as  a  block-,  saying,  "Eat ! " 

In  the  last  week  of  the  seventeenth  month  (513th  day)  she 
showed  me  some  daffodils,  asking,  "Ea'?  "  I  had  just  been  telling 
her  I  was  going  to  take  them  to  the  city,  and  it  is  possible  she 
thought  I  took  them  to  eat;  or  she  may  have  been  thinking  of  a 
picture  in  which  a  cow  was  eating  flowers.  "O  no,  we  don't  eat 
flowers,"  I  said.  "Did  Ruth  think  we  ate  flowers  ?"— "Laly?  " 
—  "No,  not  lady."  —  "  Mam  ?  "  —  "O  no,  not  man."  — ■  "Mamma?" 
and  so  on  through  the  family.  "  We  only  take  daffodils  to  look 
at,"  I  explained,  after  satisfying  her  that  no  member  of  the  family 
ate  them.  "Loo'?"  she  repeated,  leaning  forward  and  gazing 
intently  at  the  daffodils  according  to  a  pantomime  just  now  con- 
nected with  the  word  look;  then  listened  intently  while  I  explained 
that  she  and  mamma  and  the  rest  of  us  ate  meat,  and  potato,  and 
bread,  and  rice,  and  the  dog  ate  bones,  and  the  cow  grass  and 
flowers,  —  she  repeating  after  me  such  names  of  foods  as  struck 
her  interest.  Her  similar  inquiries  as  to  whether  the  moon  was  to 
be  eaten,  in  the  next  month,  (541st  day)  have  been  related  above 
(Sight,  p.  84).  In  this  month  (eighteenth)  she  would  often  show 
articles,  as  a  stem  of  sorrel,  and  ask  if  they  were  good  to  eat.  Yet 
I  felt  some  doubt  whether  she  understood  the  word  cat  at  this 
time:  she  used  it  of  putting  dirt  in  her  mouth,  with  no  intention 
of  swallowing;  bite  also  was  sometimes  confused  with  cat,  and 
sometimes  even  with  kiss,  as  if  the  action  as  seen,  not  as  felt,  was 
described  to  her  mind  by  the  words. 

She  tried  to  make  me  eat  for  her  amusement,  and  to  feed  the  doll 
(as  she  did  afterward  from  time  to  time,  at  first  perhaps  seriously,  then 
in  play,  even  to  the  present,  seven  years  old),  and  took  a  good  deal 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  22"J 

of  interest  in  feeding  the  pigs ;  still,  these  were  only  a  few  among  a 
hundred  experiments  and  amusements,  and  did  not  show  a  special 
curiosity  about  food  and  eating.  She  did  not  use  the  word  food 
during  the  whole  second  year,  but  lunch,  used  at  first  generally  of 
any  meal,  was  by  this  month  extended  to  mean  any  food.  Asked, 
"What  does  Ruth  eat?"  she  would  say,  "Lunch."  Urged  farther, 
"  But  what  does  Ruth  have  for  lunch?"  she  would  name  potato, 
bread,  etc.  She  took  a  great  deal  of  interest  in  going  out  and  see- 
ing things  cooked,  especially  if  she  was  to  eat  them ;  and  this  inter- 
est lasted  at  least  to  the  end  of  the  nineteenth  month.  In  the  first 
week  of  the  nineteenth  month,  she  was  once  in  a  deep  abstraction, 
still  sleepy  perhaps,  after  a  nap,  and  so  sensitive  to  any  interruption 
of  her  mood  that  she  put  up  her  lip  when  kissed;  but  when  her 
grandmother  said  something  about  a  chicken-bone  for  dinner,  and 
then  suggested  that  she  would  want  a  drink  after  the  cracker  she 
was  eating,  she  rose  slowly  from  her  abstraction,  and  first  asked  for 
the  drink,  then  said  earnestly,  as  I  took  her  in  my  arms,  "Wu'! 
.  .  Ft'  !  .  .  Bo  ' !  "  adding  one  word  after  another  with  grow- 
ing trouble,  as  she  found  I  did  not  understand.  "Find  Ruth's 
bone?  "  "  E'!  "  (yes).  She  consented  to  have  the  search  postponed 
for  some  other  matter,  then  asked  again  to  find  the  bone;  but  when 
taken  to  the  kitchen  forgot  it  in  the  favorite  amusement  of  looking 
into  all  the  pots  on  the  stove  and  recognizing  the  various  foods  in 
preparation.  She  liked  at  this  time  (nineteenth  month)  to  call  our 
attention  to  the  fact  that  she  was  eating,  saying,  "Up-eat! " 

In  the  twenty-first  month  it  was  a  favorite  joke  to  offer  to  eat 
impossible  things,  as  bits  of  stick,  asking,  "Ruth  eat?"  or  "Eat?" 
This,  I  thought,  grew  out  of  the  serious  curiosity  she  had  had  in 
earlier  months  as  to  what  things  really  were  eatable,  and  that  in 
turn  out  of  our  taking  things  out  of  her  mouth,  or  saying,  "That 
is  not  good  to  eat."  In  the  last  week  of  the  month  the  joke  took 
the  form  of  offering  such  uneatables  to  her  father  and  mother,  say- 
ing, "Mamma  eat?  papa  eat?"  and  laughing. 

In  the  first  week  of  the  twenty-third  month,  the  flies  came  about 
a  slice  of  bread  and  jelly  she  was  eating  in  the  kitchen,  and  I 
remarked,  "You  see  the  flies  like  bread  and  jelly  too."  This  inter- 
ested her  much,  and  she  began  breaking  off  bits  to  offer  them 
(though  when  she  came  to  the  end  of  her  slice,  her  generosity 
10 


22o  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

failed,  and  she  reclaimed  the  flies'  portion).  In  the  ninety-seventh 
week  (the  same  month)  she  was  very  much  interested  in  compar- 
ing her  own  dietary  with  that  of  her  new  baby  cousin. 

With  the  beginning  of  the  twenty-fourth  month,  plenty 
appeared,  —  "Ruth  had  plenty;"  this  was  used  of  other  things  as 
well  as  food,  and  so  did  not  express  merely  the  sense  of  satiety  of 
appetite.  Hungry  did  not  appear  at  all  in  the  second  year; 
"Ruth  want"  something  to  eat,  covered  the  ground  well  enough  for 
practical  purposes,  but  did  not  show  conscious  recognition  of  the 
sensation. 

Early  in  this  month,  her  grandmother  told  her  a  story  of  two 
little  girls  going  to  school,  —  the  simplest  possible  narrative  of 
their  starting  from  home  with  lunch-pails  in  hand,  playing  with 
their  mates,  reciting  their  lessons,  and  coming  home  to  their 
mammas;  it  proved  to  have  a  powerful  charm,  and  we  were  all 
called  on  to  tell  it  from  time  to  time,  even  to  the  end  of  the  third 
year.  I  suspected  by  the  time  it  had  lasted  a  few  weeks  that  its 
charm  lay  largely  in  the  lunch-pails  (or  baskets),  whose  contents 
were  carefully  described  always:  in'  the  last  week  of  the  twenty- 
fifth  month,  she  asked  for  it  as  the  "story  about  little  girl  go  school 
with  her  lunch  in  a  basket,"  and  displayed  deepened  interest  when 
lunch-time  arrived  in  the  story.  The  first  day  of  the  twenty-sixth 
month  she  asked  for  it  thus:  "Tell  story  about  little  girl  that  went 
to  school  and  ate  lunch.  And  two  cookies."  The  special  interest 
in  the  lunch  seems  to  have  faded  after  this,  and  in  the  thirty-fifth 
month,  in  retelling  the  story  herself  several  times,  she  made  very 
little  of  that  part. 

In  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  took  up  easily  the  play  at  hav- 
ing meals  with  toy  dishes,  and  it  has  always  been  a  favored  one  if 
she  has  a  companion  in  it.  In  the  twenty-ninth  she  commented  on 
the  cows  eating  grass;  and  in  the  thirtieth  played  at  giving  milk  to 
a  paper  cat.  In  the  thirty-first  she  dictated  a  letter,  and  made 
special  mention  of  what  she  had  been  eating;  and  the  chief  sub- 
stance of  a  dictated  letter  of  the  thirty-fourth  month  is:  "We  had 
some  biscuits  and  we  had  some  olives.  We  did  not  have  any  eggs 
before."  She  dictated  a  number  of  letters,  however,  in  which  food 
played  no  part. 

In  the  last  quarter  of  the  third  year  she  showed  a  good  deal  of 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  229 

curiosity  about  the  food  of  animals:  was  much  interested  in  the 
food  of  chipmunks,  thirty-fourth  month;  in  that  of  all  the  animals 
she  knew  about,  and  in  their  foraging  for  food,  told  with  the  pic- 
tures, thirty-fifth  month.  In  the  thirty-fifth  and  thirty-sixth 
months  (as  for  years  after),  she  was  fond  of  two  stories  about  chip- 
munks, one  in  which  the  chipmunk  crept  into  a  schoolroom  and  ate 
the  crumbs  from  the  children's  lunch,  the  other  in  which  the  chip- 
munks foraged  for  nuts.  She  had  in  the  thirty-sixth  month  some 
imaginary  parrots  concerning  whom  she  romanced,  and  their  food 
figured  largely  in  the  romancing. 

Just  at  the  end  of  the  third  year  I  caught  the  word  hungry  for 
the  first  time,  but  did  not  know  whether  it  was  used  with  very  clear 
meaning:  she  excused  herself  from  telling  me  something  I  asked 
her  about  at  dinner  by  saying,  "  I  can't  tell  you,  I  'm  so  hungry  now." ' 


I  saw  practically  no  evidence  that  suggestion  was  efficient  in 
increasing  or  lessening  hunger.  If  she  did  not  care  to  eat,  it  was 
not  possible  to  make  her  think  she  did.  An  exception  to  this  may 
be  found  in  the  following:  During  much  of  the  second  year,  and 
the  whole  of  the  third  year  (indeed,  the  habit  had  begun  in  the 
first  year)  she  was  fond  of  going  to  her  grandmother's  lap  to  finish 
up  a  meal;  and  it  often  happened  that  after  a  hasty  meal  in  her  own 
place,  she  would  begin  to  beg,  "Tootyboo  go  see  dear  grandma  ! " 
run  to  her  grandmother  and  ask,  "Dear  grandma,  take  up  Tootyboo !" 
and  in  her  grandmother's  lap  eat  with  relish  the  same  things  that 
had  been  rejected  in  her  own  seat. 

■Of  about  90  words  used  in  the  20th  month  by  Mrs.  Beatty's  boy,  but 
6  were  food-names,  and  no  general  word  for  food,  eating,  or  hunger  appears. 
Once  in  this  month  the  child  succeeded  in  conveying  the  idea  that  he  had  taken 
his  milk  by  saying,  "Mou',"  and  pointingto  his  mouth  to  show  where  the  con- 
tents of  the  bottle  had  gone. 

Mrs.  Wood's  boy,  from  the  15th  to  the  iSth  month,  used  the  word  mummv- 
mummy,  at  first  as  a  general  name  for  food  and  drink,  afterwards  for  drink  only; 
i,  e.,  milk  or  water.  He  used  eat  at  21  months.  Among  263  words  recorded 
at  2  years  old,  only  11  food-names  are  given. 


230  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Dietary  and  Digestion. 

first  year. 

No  food  except  mother's  milk  was  taken  till  the  138th  day  (20th  week);  then 
two  tablespoonfuls  of  cream,  in  two  tablespoonfnls  of  hot  water,  once  a  day  for 
a  few  days. 

23d  week,  the  cream  and  hot  water  once  a  day  was  returned  to,  as  a  regular 
addition  to  mother's  milk. 

24th  week,  changed  to  top-milk,  with  hot  water,  well  sweetened. 

25th  week,  the  baby  would  drink  a  little  ordinary  table-milk,  unsweetened 
and  undiluted,  from  a  glass  held  to  her  lips,  and  coaxed  to  have  it  given  her  in 
a  spoon,  but  evidently  for  mere  amusement  in  the  imitation,  not  because  she 
cared  for  it  as'food;  she  did  not  swallow  much. 

About  the  same  time  she  was  given  a  bread-crust  to  suck  and  mumble,  and 
by  the  end  of  the  Sth  month  this  was  a  regular  thing ;  but  little  nourishment 
could  have  been  obtained  from  it,  as  it  was  not  swallowed  much. 

31st  week,  a  few  spoonfuls  of  beef  soup  and  cream  were  given  her  experi- 
mentally, once ;  relished,  and  digested  well,  but  not  added  to  her  dietary,  as 
there  was  no  need  of  it. 

34th  week,  oatmeal  gruel  mixed  with  the  milk  in  her  daily  supplementary- 
meal. 

35th  week,  a  bit  of  dried  beef  once  or  twice  given  her  to  suck. 

During  the  Sth  month,  the  several  experiments  in  other  food  had  been  tried 
with  a  view  to  supplementing  the  mother's  milk  more  and  more;  during  the 
9th,  another  and  then  another  daily  nursing  was  dropped,  as  the  mother's  milk 
became  insufficient,  and  other  food  substituted,  and  sometime  early  in  the 
10th  month  the  breast  was  given  up  entirely  in  the  daytime. 

gth  month :  —  My  notes  are  not  quite  clear,  but  one  meal  daily  of  milk  and 
oatmeal  gruel,  and  one  of  milk,  in  addition  to  the  breast,  seems  to  have  been 
the  staple  of  her  food  during  the  9th  month.  Bread-crusts  were  regularly  given 
her  to  chew ;  she  now  swallowed  a  good  deal,  and  was  fond  of  them;  in  the 
37th  and  3S1I1  weeks,  she  had  pretzel  instead,  and  preferred  it.  She  now  and 
then  called  for  the  ordinary  milk  she  saw  others  drink  at  the  table,  and  drank 
it  willingly  (Jersey  milk,  once  skimmed,  — as  rich,  probably,  as  most  new 
milk,  but  unsweetened  and  unwarmed). 

10th  month:  —  From  the  beginning  of  this  month,  a  few  spoonfuls  of  beef 
soup  daily.  Crackers  were  now  given  her  instead  of  the  crusts  or  pretzels,  — 
soda  cracker  at  first,  but  graham  flake  from  the  41st  week,  and  as  she  had 
little  desire  for  the  milk,  or  milk  and  gruel,  that  made  the  staple  of  her  food,  the 
graham  crackers  were  an  important  addition  to  her  diet ;  she  ate  a  whole  one 
with  each  meal,  and  would  not  take  her  milk  without. 

From  the  6th  month,  the  baby  had  been  allowed  to  taste  a  little  fruit-juice 
now  and  then  :  in  the  9th  month ,  perhaps  once  or  twice  a  week,  —  loquat  and 
orange  fresh  from  the  tree.  In  the  10th  month,  she  was  allowed  it  more  fre- 
quently, and  in  increased  quantity,  (peach  and  loquat,)  so  that  it  may  count  as 
an  appreciable  addition  to  her  diet. 

41st  week,  a  little  bread  soaked  with  milk. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  231 

42d  week,  oatmeal  mush.  She  had  been  more  and  more  averse  to  the  oat- 
meal gruel  in  milk,  and  as  this  and  milk,  which  she  cared  little  for,  were  the 
staple  of  her  food,  it  was  difficult  to  get  her  to  take  nourishment  enough,  and 
the  doctor  advised  giving  the  oatmeal  in  the  more  solid  form.  At  the  same 
time,  baked  apple  was  added  to  her  regular  food.  Toast  soaked  in  milk  was 
also  tried  this  week. 

The  effect  of  this  more  varied  and  solid  diet  seemed  almost  wholly  good  : 
loss  of  weight  stopped  (p.  7,  above),  appetite  revived  and  food  was  more 
enjoyed  than  ever  before  in  the  baby's  life,  and  a  persistent  habit  of  constipation 
relaxed  at  once  ;  the  oatmeal,  however,  did  not  always  digest  perfectly. 
Accordingly, 

43d  week,  a  wheat  preparation  ("germea")  substituted  for  oatmeal. 

Graham  crackers  and  milk  continued  a  regular  part  of  the  baby's  food  all 
the  time. 

/////  month :  —  44th  week,  oatmeal  mush  was  given  in  the  morning,  and 
another  wheat  preparation  ("  wheatina  ")  in  the  afternoon,  the  baby  having  be- 
come quite  averse  to  the  germea.  The  oatmeal  once  a  day  seemed  to  digest 
well. 

45th  week,  apple-sauce  (stewed)  instead  of  baked  apple.  Cream  was  given 
with  the  apple  sauce. 

46th  week,  tired  of  graham  cracker,  and  preferred  bread  and  toast  crusts 
again.     Nursing  at  night  was  given  up  this  week,  weaning  thus  being  completed. 

47th  week,  the  wheatina  given  up,  as  the  child  became  averse  to  it.  She 
cannot  be  persuaded  to  eat  more  than  three  meals  a  day:  breakfast  of  oatmeal 
about  7:30  or  8;  lunch,  of  baked  apple  and  cream,  graham  cracker  or  bread 
crust,  at  12  or  12:30;  supper  of  the  same  about  5  o'clock,  milk  with  each  meal. 
At  6:30,  when  the  family  dine,  she  gets  a  little  more  food,  —  a  few  spoonfuls  of 
soup,  a  little  milk,  a  crust  nibbled  at  a  little,  e.  g.  The  nourishment  she  was 
getting  seemed  quite  insufficient,  and  her  indifference  to  food  was  again  so 
persistent  that,  on  the  doctor's  advice,  a  little  minced  steak  and  potato  was 
tried,  but  was  not  much  liked  ;  the  next  day,  beef  broth  at  noon,  which  was 
fairly  well  liked,  but  the  baby  had  little  appetite  for  it  after  a  few  mouthfuls. 

Baked  potato  and  cream  was  also  added  to  the  afternoon  meal,  and  a  little 
rice  from  the  soup  was  tried.  A  cup  of  milk  at  midnight  was  given,  in  place  of 
the  nursing. 

48th  week,  she  was  given  a  bit  of  steak  to  suck,  with  the  idea  that  she 
would  simply  get  the  juice  ;  but  she  cared  only  to  bite  and  tear  it,  and  swallowed 
more  or  less  of  it.  It  seemed  to  do  her  no  harm,  but  care  was  taken  the  next  time 
(a  few  days  after)  to  give  her  a  piece  too  tough  to  tear;  this  annoyed  her,  and 
she  would  not  suck  it. 

Throughout  this  nth  month,  fruit  had  been  given  almost  daily  to  suck, — 
the  fruit  tied  in  a  thin  cloth,  through  which  only  the  juice  could  be  got.  I  note 
on  one  day,  in  the  45th  week,  that  the  juice  of  four  loquats  was  thus  sucked ; 
on  another,  in  the  48th  week,  that  the  juice  of  one  peach  was  taken  in  the 
course  of  the  day.  Watermelon  was  frequently  sucked  in  this  way.  In  one 
case,  she  somehow  swallowed  some  of  the  pulp  of  an  orange,  which  failed  to 
digest. 


2 3-  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

I  have  no  note  of  any  harm  to  health  or  comfort  that  was  ever  apparent 
from  the  occasional  passage  of  food  through  her  stomach  thus  undigested  ; 
not  even  from  the  bits  of  chewed  paper,  leaves,  etc.,  that  now  and  then,  in  spite 
of  much  vigilance,  were  found  to  have  taken  the  same  course. 

12th  month :  —  49th  week,  sweet  potato'  tried,  but  she  would  only  muss  it 
around,  and  would  not  eat.  The  oatmeal  was  now  refused,  and  wheatina, 
baked  pear,  soup,  cracker,  and  milk,  made  most  of  her  diet. 

50th  week,  returned  to  oatmeal  mush,  with  milk,  bread,  graham  crackers, 
and  toast.  Baked  apples  eaten  somewhat.  A  bit  of  steak  tried  again,  and  as 
before,  she  tore  and  swallowed  it ;  but  as  it  seemed  to  agree  with  her  well,  and 
as  the  doctor  so  advised,  she  was  allowed  to  go  on  doing  this.  As  a  sample 
of  her  lunch  at  this  time,  I  note  an  inch  and  a  half  of  steak,  about  four  inches  of 
bread,  and  a  little  baked  apple,  with  milk.  Mush  and  toast  at  breakfast,  soup 
at  night,  were  continued. 

Fruit-juice  was  taken  as  in  the  previous  month,  and  to  this  was  added  in  the 
50th  week  a  little  apple  flesh,  scraped  fine. 

SECOND   YEAR. 

ijt/i  month :  —  Sweet  potato  forms  a  principal  part  of  her  diet,  and  is  greatly 
liked  ;  the  366th  day  she  ate  a  whole  one  for  lunch.  With  slight  variation,  her 
dietary  this  month  is  mush  and  milk  toast  in  the  morning  ;  sweet  potato  at  noon, 
and  twice  or  three  times  a  week  a  bit  of  rare  broiled  steak  (perhaps  three 
square  inches,  half  or  three-quarters  of  an  inch  thick)  ;  soup  and  sweet  potato 
at  night,  with  milk,  bread,  and  crackers  at  every  meal,  and  often  apple  sauce  or 
baked  apple.  A  light  lunch  of  cracker  and  milk  in  the  middle  of  the  afternoon, 
and  a  cup  of  milk  in  the  night.  Fruit  almost  every  day.  375th  day,  sucked  a 
chicken-bone,  getting  some  bits  of  meat  from  it. 

This  dietary  seems  to  agree  with  her  well,  her  appetite  for  the  grain  foods 
and  milk  improving  when  a  little  meat  was  added  to  them. 

jjth  month  : — Irish  potato  and  bread,  instead  of  sweet  potato  and  graham 
cracker,  neither  of  which  is  now  liked  ;  offered  graham  cracker,  she  will  bite  it 
and  drop  it.  Food  now  largely  Irish  potato  ;  a  whole  one  eaten  for  dinner  the 
403d  day.  With  these  substitutions,  the  dietary  much  the  same  as  in  the  13th 
month,  viz.,  milk  and  potato  with  all  meals,  besides  mush  and  toast  at  break- 
fast, bread  at  lunch,  bread,  soup,  and  occasionally  apple-sauce  and  cracker, 
jelly  and  butter  on  bread.  Salt  now  used  more,  especially  on  potato,  and 
relished  extremely. 

Rice  is  added  in  the  37th  week,  and  much  liked.  Celery  now  first  eaten 
(60th  week),  and  passionately  desired,  a  taste  that  lasted  at  least  until  the  19th 
month  ;  but  it  never  formed  any  considerable  part  of  her  food,  though  she  was 
often  allowed  to  eat  a  little. 

15th  month: — Much  the  same  as  foregoing  :  lives  mainly  on  mush,  potato, 
toast,  bread,  and  milk,  with  baked  or  stewed  apple.  Meat  three  or  four  times 
a  week,  at  lunch,  or  rarely  at  dinner,  —  steak  or  chicken.  Meat  is  more  eagerly 
desired  than  any  other  food.  A  cooky  (plain)  now  and  then  given,  but  forms 
no  material  part  of  food. 

16th  month  :  —  Same  as  above:  but  graham  crackers  again  important  part  of 
food.     Begs  for  meat  at  every  meal,  but  accepts  refusal  easily. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  233 

17th  month: — Same,  but  rice  more  important  part  of  food.  No  longer 
clamors  for  meat  except  at  the  time  it  is  regularly  given.  This  month  careless 
habit  in  the  family  of  giving  her  things  to  eat  irregularly  when  she  asks  for  them, 
oranges,  cracker,  candy.  (Candy  first  mentioned  as  given  for  hiccoughs  in 
15th  month ;  never  much  more  than  tasted  till  now.) 

i8tk  month:  —  Same.  In  the  76th  week,  two  nights  sleepless  and  crying, 
which  we  credited  (though  she  had  a  cough)  to  the  eating  between  meals  ;  this 
was  stopped,  and  her  sleep  became  quiet. 

Fruit  was  freely  eaten  all  this  half-year,  (apples  and  oranges,)  always  agree- 
ing with  her  well. 

igth  -  24th  months :  —  The  general  dietary  was  the  same  as  in  the  former 
half-year.  In  the  19th  month,  especially  eager  for  chicken,  and  would  ask  for 
more  at  table.  In  the  20th,  graham  bread  much  favored  instead  of  white;  a 
bit  of  mutton-chop  once  at  least  at  lunch.  A  fit  of  nausea  the  next  day,  without 
apparent  cause,  was  charged  to  the  mutton,  and  I  find  no  farther  note  of  her 
having  any,  though  she  asked  for  it  several  times  in  the  next  month.  21st 
month,  at  last  indifferent  to  steak;  tired  also  of  potato  and  rice.  Egg  tried 
for  the  first  time,  and  liked.  23d  month,  sweet  potato  and  chicken  favored 
again. 

During  the  whole  half-year,  fruit  was  a  chief  article  of  diet,  always  agreeing 
with  her  except  once  in  the  81st  week  when  some  one  gave  her  half-ripe  loquats, 
skins  and  all.  Through  the  21st  and  22d  months,  fruit  was  preferred  to  all 
other  food,  and  potato,  bread  and  rice  were  cleared  away  from  her  plate  with 
industry  in  order  to  reach  peach  or  berries.  Most  of  the  time,  in  these  two 
months,  cared  little  for  any  other  food,  and  it  was  impressed  on  her  as  a  great 
duty  to  eat  bread  or  potato  before  asking  for  fruit.  Strawberries,  raspberries, 
loquats,  peaches,  watermelon,  chiefly  mentioned. 

THIRD   YEAR. 

Same  staple  articles  in  the  main  as  in  the  second  year.  A  few  additions,  — 
string  beans  in  25th  month  ;  roast  beef  at  some  time  early  in  the  first  quarter- 
year  ;  corn  bread  sometime  in  the  same  period  ;  olives  (ripe)  in  the  28th  month; 
codfish  in  the  30th  ;  battercake  some  time  before  the  34th,  and  apparently  also 
biscuit  (American  sense,  of  course, —  light  roll);  summer  squash,  and  fresh 
and  canned  corn  ;  duck  in  36th  month.  Probably  some  other  things  were 
allowed,  not  mentioned  here,  for  the  record  is  not  complete,  as  in  the  first 
and  second  year ;  other  fresh  vegetables  especially:  ham  and  baked  beans 
I  find  mentioned  as  articles  expressly  forbidden.  Meat  was  eaten  daily 
in  the  third  year,  but  no  longer  so  much  preferred  to  other  food  as  during 
most  of  the  second  year:  in  the  25th  month  I  mention  that  chicken  is 
neglected  for  string  beans ;  in  the  26th  that  fruit  is  more  desired  than  meat ; 
in  the  27th  that  fondness  for  steak  has  declined,  but  roast  beef  is  liked 
as  well  as  ever;  in  the  30th,  that  chicken  is  consistently  refused  for 
some  days,  and  in  general,  taste  for  meat  has  declined,  and  bread  and  jelly  is 
the  staple  of  diet;  in  the  32d,  meat,  fruit,  and  sweets  are  greatly  desired,  and 
would  be  the  exclusive  food  if  permitted.  From  the  30th  month  milk  was 
taken  reluctantly,  and  in  the  32d  refused,  and  water  asked  for  instead.  Fruit 
taken  all  the  year,  —  grapes  especially,  in  25th  month. 


234  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Of  articles  not  staples  of  diet,  I  find  the  following  added  in  the  third  year : 
25th  month,  cake,  almonds  (rarely,  —  one  a  day  in  the  latter  part  of  the  year), 
and  dates  ;  2bth  month,  figs ;  28th  month,  bananas  ;  30th,  popcorn;  36th,  dried 
prunes. 

Digestion  seemed  in  the  main  perfect  throughout  the  year:  just  at  the  25th 
month,  there  was  a  period  of  languid  appetite,  accompanied  with  signs  of  cold; 
in  the  last  week  of  the  31st  month,  early  raspberries,  though  perfectly  ripe, 
upset  her  stomach,  and  were  vomited,  undigested,  24  hours  after  they  were 
eaten;  in  the  33d  month,  an  older  child  gave  her  large  quantities  of  cherries 
and  loquats,  without  care  as  to  ripeness,  with  the  result  of  another  fit  of  indiges- 
tion and  vomiting;  in  the  36th  month,  there  was  again  a  little  spell  of  languid 
appetite,  connected  with  some  feverishness  and  fretfulness,  and  in  the  last  days 
of  the  month  another  digestive  upset,  apparently  from  eating  figs,  though 
these  seemed  to  agree  with  her  as  a  rule. 

2.  Thirst. 

During  the  first  year,  when  milk  formed  so  large  a  part  of  the 
food,  I  did  not  especially  note  the  desire  for  water:  pains  was 
always  taken  from  the  first,  however,  to  see  that  the  baby  was 
offered  water  frequently,  and  especially  when  she  was  restless  with- 
out obvious  cause.  Both  the  doctor  and  the  grandmother  were 
earnest  on  this  point,  saying  that  little  babies  desired  a  great  deal  of 
water,  and  suffered  much  discomfort  and  harm  for  lack  of  it  before 
they  could  ask.  As  soon  as  the  child  could  ask  for  water,  she 
began  to  do  so,  and  I  noted  that  at  a  year  old  she  drank  a  great  deal  of 
water,  and  craved  it.  From  that  time  on,  I  noticed  that  her  craving  for 
water  was  remarkable.  In  the  thirty-third  month,  she  would  not 
drink  milk  and  invariably  asked  for  water  instead,  and  drank  a  great 
deal.  At  meals  she  would  ask  for  more  water  over  and  over,  and  at 
night  always  wanted  water;  one  night  I  noticed  that  she  asked  for  it 
three  times  in  a  half  hour,  though  not  feverish.  The  demands  of 
little  children,  night  and  day,  for  water,  fairly  try  the  patience  of 
those  in  charge,  and  it  is  probable  that  for  health  and  growth  they 
need  more  water  than  it  is  easy  for  an  adult  to  realize.  The  evap- 
oration from  their  skin  (Prof.  Le  Conte  suggests  to  me)  is  much 
greater  than  from  ours,  owing  to  its  soft  and  porous  texture. 

3.  Nausea. 

Preyer  seems  to  have  found  no  evidence  of  the  existence  of 
nausea  in  the  first  three  years.  My  niece  showed  it  distinctly  sev- 
eral   times.     On    the    few  occasions  in  the  earliest    months    when 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  235 

regurgitation  of  food  occurred,  it  is  true,  there  was  not  the  least 
sign  of  nausea.  But  in  the  tenth  month  (299th  day)  the  mother, 
chancing  to  be  where  there  were  some  pleasant  warm  sulphur  baths, 
took  the  baby  into  them.  Shortly  after,  the  baby  was  taken  with 
every  appearance  of  violent  nausea,  with  vomiting,  which  recurred 
all  day.  No  other  instance  occurred  till  the  eighty-first  week,  when 
vomiting  was  caused  by  some  green  fruit,  with  the  skin:  nausea 
seemed  to  accompany  this,  and  the  next  day  when  the  child  was 
asked  where  she  was  sick  yesterday,  she  placed  her  hands  at  once 
on  her  stomach,  then  doubtfully  on  her  chest  and  around  her  sides, 
toward  her  back.  Asked  a  second  time,  she  placed  her  hands  as 
before,  first  decidedly  on  her  stomach,  then  hesitantly  backward. 
No  one  had  told  her  anything  about  the  location  of  the  trouble, 
and  she  evidently  recalled  and  correctly  located  either  the  nausea 
or  the  muscular  sensation  of  vomiting.  In  the  eighty-third  week- 
she  was  again  sick  several  times;  and  this  time  she  announced  the 
nausea  each  time  before  vomiting  by  running  to  her  mother  and 
crying,  "Baby  sick!"  —  an  unmistakable  evidence  that  she  under- 
stood and  named  the  feeling.  The  next  day,  asked  where  she  had 
been  sick,  she  said  "stomach,"  and  put  her  hand  on  the  right  place. 
Three  times  in  the  third  year  she  suffered  similar  small  upsets 
from  indiscretions  in  eating;  the  second  time  (thirty-third  month)  it 
left  her  droopy  and  sleepy,  an  effect  that  had  not  been  seen  before. 
In  the  last  week  of  the  thirty-fifth  month,  she  had  a  bad  fall  which 
caused  a  slight  concussion  of  the  brain:  she  was  somewhat  nau- 
seated at  once,  and  after  an  hour  became  very  sick,  and  vomited, 
then  sleepy ;  she  could  not  be  persuaded  to  eat,  but  drank  a  little 
milk;  next  day,  she  would  eat  only  a  little  toast,  and  did  not  wish 
her  breakfast,  —  but  these  effects  were  of  course  due  to  other 
causes  than  the  simple  nausea. 

4.  Other  Organic  Sensations. 

The  child  repeatedly  choked  a  little  in  nursing  in  the  first  six 
months,  and  once  or  twice  when  she  had  kicked  from  her  mother's 
hands  in  the  bath  and  gone  under  water;  but  she  showed  no  sign  of 
especial  fright  or  discomfort.  In  the  seventy-eighth  week,  how- 
ever, she  choked  on  a  bit  of  candy;  and  though  she  succeeded  at 
once  in  raising  it  from  her  throat  when  told  to  "spit  it  out,"  she 


236  University  of  Califot'nia.  [Vol.  i. 

found  the  sensation  alarming  and  distressing  while  it  lasted;  began 
to  cry  as  soon  as  she  felt  it  and  showed  considerable  relief  when 
it  ceased.  In  the  87th  week,  choking  on  a  drink,  she  merely  re- 
marked gayly,  "Too  mil'  gi' !"     Too  much  drink.) 

Except  in  going  under  the  water  a  few  seconds,  and  in  difficult 
breathing  from  a  cold,  the  sensation  of  suffocation  was  never 
experienced  that  I  know:  but  it  is  quite  possible  that  some  such 
sensation,  coming  from  disturbances  of  respiration,  may  cause  the 
unaccountable  crying  of  babies,  especially  on  waking  from  sleep, 
as  it  has  to  do  with  the  vague  horror  experienced  in  nightmare 
with  us. 

The  single  note  I  made  of  organic  pain  (colic)  has  already  been 
given  (Pain,  p.  154,  footnote). 


GENERAL  SENSATION, 
i.  Sensations  of  Well-being  and  Discomfort. 

Professor  Preyer  intimates '  that  in  the  first  months  the  infant 
must  experience  a  larger  proportion  of  unpleasant  than  of  pleasant 
sensation.  This  does  not  accord  with  my  observation;  and  the 
opinion  of  the  best  nursery  authority  I  have  consulted  is  emphat- 
ically to  the  effect  that  if  a  healthy  baby  is  not  in  the  main  a  happy 
one  from  the  first,  the  care  it  receives  is  at  fault.  But  there  are 
certainly  individual  differences  between  babies,  some  of  whom 
appear,  under  conditions  of  general  health  and  comfort,  to  experience 
a  lively  felicity,  and  others  a  merely  neutral  feeling.  So,  too,  some 
babies  are  less  troubled  than  others  by  actual  discomforts,  and 
recover  from  them  promptly,  instead  of  seeming  for  some  time  after- 
ward affected  by  disagreeable  reverberations.2 

In  the  case  of  my  niece  the  underlying  condition  of  sensation 
seems  to  have  been  usually  agreeable.  It  is  true  that  in  the  first 
week  or  two,  besides  some  fretting  for  special  reasons  (as  hunger, 
or  slight  touches  of  colic),  a  vague  discomfort,  not  enough  to  cause 
fretting,  sometimes  appeared  (see  p.  180,  above);  while  positive 
expressions  of  comfort  were  not  noted,  ■ —  her  prevailing  feeling,  to 
judge  by  her  face,  being  quite  neutral.     But  with  the  second  week, 

'"The  unpleasant  feelings  predominate  until  sleep  interrupts  them." — The 
Senses  and  the  Will,  p.  143. 

-There  is  room  here  for  interesting  speculation  on  the  connection  between 
temperament  and  idiosyncrasies  of  general  sensation.  We  may  surmise,  for 
instance,  that  a  happy  temperament,  heeding  discomforts  slightly  and  recover- 
ing from  them  quickly,  is  due  to  a  constant  undercurrent  of  agreeable  general 
sensation,  which  modifies  disagreeable  special  sensations,  and  quickly  regains 
dominance  in  consciousness,  obliterating  any  faint  remnants  of  discomfort;  that 
the  converse  is  true  in  the  case  of  a  melancholy  temperament;  while  a  temper- 
ament that  feels  both  pleasure  and  pain  strongly  and,  so  to  speak,  with  the 
whole  consciousness,  is  one  in  which  the  undercurrent  of  general  sensation  is 
for  some  reason  unstable,  and  instead  of  modifying  the  force  of  special 
impressions,  is  readily  deflected  into  the  same  course  with  these,  strengthening 
and  prolonging  them. 

(  237  ) 


23S  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

the  grimaces  of  vague  discomfort  disappeared,  and  an  expression  of 
great  contentment  became  apparent  when  the  baby  was  warm,  dry, 
fed,  and  wide-awake.  It  is  worthy  of  note  that  this  expression  of 
positive  content  appeared  in  close  sequence  after  the  habit  of  staring 
at  bright  surfaces,  as  if  the  material,  so  to  speak,  for  the  diffusion  of 
agreeable  general  sensation  began  with  the  action  of  the  special 
senses.  From  this  time  on,  while  there  are  daily  notes  of  fretting 
for  this  or  that,  there  are  also  daily  notes  of  visible  contentment  and 
well-being.  The  first  true  smiles  (latter  part  of  the  first  month) 
were  expressions  of  this  general  comfort,  not  of  special  pleasure; 
and  with  the  second  month,  happy  sounds  began  to  be  uttered  at 
these  times  of  contentment.  Up  to  the  eighth  week,  there  was 
always  a  good  deal  of  vague  movement  of  limbs  and  head  (see  p. 
180,  above)  at  such  times.  I  have  noted  above  the  suddenness 
with  which  in  the  second  month  the  pleasant  mood  gave  place  to 
fretting. 

Activity  of  attention  and  interest  was  heightened  at  times  of 
general  well-being,  so  that  it  now  became  hard  always  to  discern 
whether  the  joy  displayed  was  due  to  the  sense  of  well-being  itself, 
or  to  pleasure  in  the  objects  of  attention.  Already,  in  the  second 
month,  when  lying  warm,  fed,  and  altogether  comfortable,  the  baby 
took  great  interest  in  the  faces  of  those  bending  over  her,  and 
smiled  at  them  in  much  gayety.  In  the  third  month  her  perpetual 
desire  to  be  sitting  up  interfered  with  moods  of  contentment,  till  it 
was  gratified;  then  she  would  sit  happily,  propped  with  cushions, 
many  minutes  at  a  time,  playing  with  a  rattle  and  crowing.  In  the 
fourth  month  (especially  when  propped  in  her  high-chair  at  table, 
where  the  lights  and  the  rattling  of  dishes  supplied  additional  stimu- 
lus) the  faces  and  voices  about  her  made  her  display  the  most  viva- 
cious delight,  with  smiles  and  movements,  cooing  and  crowing.  In 
the  fifth  month  pleasure  in  faces  and  voices  clearly  excited  expressions 
of  joy  oftener  and  more  intensely  than  the  mere  sense  of  well-being. 
Yet  I  find  a  good  many  notes,  in  the  fourth  and  fifth  months,  of 
similar  jubilant  behavior  when  she  lay  by  herself  on  bed  or  floor, 
kicking,  crowing,  smiling,  and  murmuring,  delighted  if  paid  attention 
to,  and  happy  if  not.  As  a  rule,  some  occupation,  something  to 
handle  and  look  at,  was  necessary  to  contentment,  —  at  least  a 
rattle  to  flourish  and  to  put  into  her  mouth:  but  I  have  notes  of 


Shinn.j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  239 

two  instances  at  least  when  she  lay  in  bed,  perfectly  quiet  and 
happy  and  wide-awake,  quite  alone  and  unoccupied,  for  a  long  time, 
saying  "agoo"  peaceably  to  herself,  smiling  when  I  bent  over  her, 
but  content  to  lie  still  as  she  was.  This  placid  contentment,  how- 
ever, was  now  rare,  and  active  jollity  more  common. 

Throughout  the  rest  of  the  first  half-year  my  notes  record,  over 
and  over,  her  moods  of  "jollity,"  "perfect  content,"  and  "high 
spirits."  The  138th  day  I  speak  of  jollity  as  the  baby's  normal 
condition;  and  such  notes  recur  constantly  as,  "In  highest  spirits 
all  breakfast  time,  laughing  with  glee  when  any  one  smiled  or  spoke 
to  her;"  "Very  happy,  smiling  about  and  murmuring;"  "Lay  look- 
ing at  us  and  uttering  contented  murmurs;"  "Exceedingly jolly, 
smiling,  kicking,  and  sputtering;"  "In  highest  spirits,  frequently 
giggling,  ■ — ■  if  her  mother  pointed  a  finger  at  her  and  said,  'O 
Ruth ! '  she  would  giggle."  A  characteristic  of  this  condition  of 
spirits  seemed  to  be  that  she  was  easily  "set  off"  into  merriment 
by  a  word  or  touch.  I  noted  this  first  about  the  end  of  the  fourth 
month,  when  smiles,  or  even  laughter  and  joyous  movements,  could 
be  coaxed  at  almost  any  time,  in  these  sunny  moods,  by  a  few 
caressing  words  and  touches ;  and  the  same  thing  will  be  seen  in 
the  notes  below,  in  later  months. 

In  these  moods,  also,  the  baby  was  apt  to  coax  for  a  frolic.  The 
connection  between  general  joyousness  of  mood,  and  joy  in  muscu- 
lar activity  and  in  motion  (spoken  of  quite  fully  above),  was  close, 
and  it  was  not  always  easy  to  tell  which  inspired  the  other. 

Throughout  most  of  the  second  half-year,  also,  my  notes  read, 
day  after  day,  "  Rolls  about  by  the  hour  on  the  floor,  kicking  and 
crowing ;  "  "  Very  happy  all  day,  rolling  about  the  floor,  kicking 
and  prattling  ;  "  "  Especially  happy  most  of  the  day,  answering  with 
laughter  when  spoken  to."  Besides  the  pure  joy  in  existence  that 
seemed  to  fill  her,  her  pleasure  in  her  own  increasing  freedom  of 
muscular  action  and  sense  activity,  her  delight  in  motion  and  frolic, 
seemed  to  fill  her  days  with  an  exuberant  joyousness.  Laughter, 
shouts  of  joy,  overflowing  ejaculations  of  happiness,  delight  and 
pride,  deep  and  happy  interest,  are  constantly  recorded  in  connec- 
tion with  special  pleasures,  such  as  the  use  of  her  own  bodily 
powers,  exploring  and  investigating,  the  more  varied  dietary  of  the 
latter   part    of  the   year,  the   daily  reappearance    of  friends    after 


240  University  of  California.  [vol.  i. 

absence,  etc.  Most  of  the  time,  she  wanted  to  play  instead  of 
sleeping  or  eating. 

In  the  last  two  months  of  the  year,  notes  of  a  general  joyousness, 
apart  from  specific  pleasures,  diminish.  The  baby's  appetite  was 
not  good  in  the  eleventh  month,  and  her  health  was  not  perfect ; 
still,  the  absence  of  notes  of  general  joyousness  seems  to  be  due  not 
so  much  to  any  real  lowering  in  the  level  of  her  spirits,  as  to  the 
fact  that  her  increasing  power  of  occupying  herself  was  so  filling  her 
time  with  special  pleasures  that  her  merriment  was  all  credited  to 
these.  She  would  play  about  on  the  floor  with  frequent  little 
shouts,  crows,  squeals,  and  babble  of  joy  ;  she  rejoiced  to  go  from 
one  room  to  another,  shouting  and  calling;  her  ecstasy  when  taken 
out  driving  is  described  below  in  connection  with  the  love  of  out- 
doors. In  the  twelfth  month,  as  she  became  still  more  absorbed  in 
her  play,  there  was  an  increasing  seriousness  of  curiosity  and 
attention,  and  a  corresponding  decline  of  gayety. 

In  the  second  and  third  years  mere  physical  gayety  was  more 
and  more  obscured  by  the  increasing  complexity  of  the  child's 
occupations  and  interests;  and  my  notes  also,  being  more  and  more 
taxed  with  phases  of  mental  development,  fail  to  keep  as  close 
record  of  physical  conditions  as  in  the  first  year.  Few  months 
pass,  however,  without  notes  of  the  liveliest  gayety:  the  child 
would  run  about  with  joyous  little  cries,  prattling,  singing,  breaking 
into  occasional  small  shouts  and  capers  in  sheer  exuberance  of 
spirits,  jumping,  racing,  tossing  her  arms,  and  in  every  way  show- 
ing the  utmost  physical  joyousness. 

Of  general  moods  of  depression  breaking  this  level  of  high 
spirits,  I  find  little  mention,  apart  from  evident  physical  indisposi- 
tion, or  from  the  conditions  attending  sleep  (see  below,  pp.  261,  279). 
The  first  instance  I  find  is  on  the  57th  day,  when  the  baby  was 
very  silent,  looking  soberly  about,  and  fretting  to  be  held  sitting. 
An  episode  of  apparently  causeless  discontent  in  the  fifth  month 
(137th  and  138th  days)  proved  to  be  due  to  insufficient  nourishment 
(p.  217).  Once  in  the  sixth  month  (173d  day)  I  find  the  note, 
"Well  and  happy,  though  not  in  overflowing  spirits,  as  some 
preceding  days;  only  quietly  pleased  at  frolics,  and  did  not  want 
to  be  laid  down  to  kick."     For  about  four  days  in  the  latter  part  of 


shinn.]  '       The  Development  of  a   Child.  241 

the  eighth  month,  she  was  sober,  and  did  not  roll  and  rollick  as 
much  as  usual;  she  laughed  and  played  a  good  deal,  but  the 
perfect  joyousness  was  tempered.  One  of  these  days  was  cold  and 
drizzly,  and  she  was  kept  in  the  house,  which  grew  close;  at  last  I 
wrapped  her  up  and  took  her  out,  and  she  came  back  with  all 
depression  dissipated.  Half  a  dozen  instances  of  more  decided 
general  discomfort  are  noted  (fifth  to  eleventh  months):  for  several 
hours  she  would  be  restless,  fretful,  ready  to  cry  on  slight  occasion> 
and  would  demand  attention  and  diversion  constantly.  These 
instances  all  occurred  within  the  first  period  of  dentition,  (which 
was  also  that  of  gradual  weaning,)  and  in  at  least  two  cases  the 
connection  with  dentition  was  evident:  once,  in  the  sixth  month, 
the  baby  had  been  restless  and  fretful  on  two  successive  afternoons, 
and  by  evening  had  cried  hard  unless  constantly  diverted,  but  from 
the  hour  the  tooth  (the  first  one)  came  through  she  was  happy 
and  smiling;  and  again  in  the  ninth  month  there  was  an  immediate 
return  of  good  spirits  after  the  appearance  of  a  tooth.  In  neither 
case  had  there  been  any  evidence  of  local  pain  before  the  cutting  of 
the  tooth,  but  rather  of  a  general  discomfort.  It  is  likely  that  there 
really  was  local  pain,  not  localized  by  the  baby;  even  grown  people 
find  it  hard  sometimes  to  locate  a  dull  pain:  but  the  general 
disorders  that  sometimes  attend  teething,  show  that  there  may 
easily  be  very  diffused  states  of  uncomfortable  sensation  at  this 
time.  The  few  remaining  instances  of  fretfulness  preceded  the 
appearance  of  teeth  by  sufficient  intervals  to  be  referred  to  early 
stages  of  their  growth:  it  is  an  old  doctrine  that  there  are  two 
periods  in  the  growth  of  each  tooth  when  it  causes  distress,  the 
first  when  it  starts  to  push  up  from  the  bone,  the  second  when 
it  is  about  to  break  through  the  skin.  Still,  most  of  the  teeth 
appeared  without  any  disturbance  of  the  baby's  spirits  that  was 
marked  enough  to  get  into  my  notes. 

In  the  second  and  third  years,  also,  I  find  only  four  times  a  note 
that  the  child  is  fretful  and  exigeant,  contrary,  or  easily  grieved, 
when  she  seems  perfectly  well ;  and  on  two  of  these  occasions 
a  failure  of  appetite  indicates  the  connection  of  the  mood  with 
obscure  physical  derangements.  Beyond  this,  "bad  days,"  "getting 
up  wrong  side  of  the  bed,"  etc.,  do  not  occur.  There  were  long 
periods  (to  be  spoken  of  presently)  of  a  perceptible  lowering  in  the 


242  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

level  of  spirits,  without  falling  below  that  of  cheerfulness  and  a 
general  habit  of  enjoyment.  Other  variations  of  mood  had  a  clear 
relation  to  physical  disorder,  —  in  a  few  cases,  to  slight  digestive 
disturbance,  but  oftenest,  as  in  the  first  year,  to  dentition.  The 
cutting  of  a  tooth  was  apt  to  be  preceded  (though  not  always) 
by  perhaps  a  half-day  of  fretfulness  and  general  sensitiveness,  — 
which  in  two  cases  disappeared,  just  as  I  had  twice  noticed  in 
the  first  year,  immediately  on  the  appearance  of  the  tooth,  and 
was  followed  by  a  mood  of  positive  joyousness.  One  of  these 
instances  was  in  the  eighteenth  month:  the  child  was  in  the 
morning  unusually  exigeant,  and  in  the  afternoon  fretful,  especially 
as  it  was  drizzly  and  she  could  not  go  outdoors;  she  begged 
hard  and  persistently  to  go,  and  could  not  easily  be  diverted. 
Thinking  to  interest  her,  I  showed  her  her  crib  and  chair,  moved 
out  on  the  veranda  in  the  process  of  house-cleaning:  instead  of 
being  pleased  at  the  novelty,  however,  she  was  greatly  distressed 
over  the  exile  of  her  belongings,  and  was  only  kept  from  wailing 
over  the  dismembered  crib  by  seeing  her  chair,  at  least,  rescued 
from  the  miscellany  of  furniture  and  brought  to  her.  After  dinner 
her  mood  changed  suddenly:  she  was  jolly  and  happy,  ran  about 
in  great  spirits,  uttering  many  inarticulate  exclamations  of  happi- 
ness, and  asking  no  attention,  and  took  lively  and  pleasant  interest 
in  the  re-laying  of  carpet  and  replacing  of  furniture,  rejoicing 
especially  over  her  crib.  It  proved  that  a  tooth,  a  molar,  had 
come  through  the  skin  late  in  the  day.  Again,  in  the  nineteenth 
month,  she  had  for  a  day  or  two  showed  some  crossness,  but 
immediately  after  the  appearance  of  an  eye-tooth  had  a  fit  of  the 
highest  spirits,  running  about  with  joyous  little  cries,  capering, 
shouting,  scrambling  on  the  lounge  and  off,  in  irrepressible  glee, 
which  remained  her  general  mood  for  at  least  four  days.  In  a 
third  case  (nineteenth  month)  the  cutting  of  a  tooth  was  followed 
on  the  next  day  by  a  fit  of  wild  spirits.  The  child  had  a  good 
many  colds  in  the  two  years;  but  I  find  only  four  instances  in 
which  peevishness  or  languor  is  associated  with  one,  and  a  dozen 
in  which  her  excellent  spirits,  in  spite  of  a  severe  cold,  are  men- 
tioned. Thus  in  the  latter  part  of  the  twenty-fifth  month,  she  had 
a  tenacious  cold,  which  did  not  affect  her  spirits  appreciably  for 
some  days;  then,  having  one  day  a  little  more  cold,  she  became 


shi.nn.j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  243 

very  irritable,  and  whimpered  because  her  dress  sleeves  did  not 
come  down  as  far  as  her  shirt  sleeves ;  because  her  hair  was  brushed 
back;  because  her  mother  instead  of  aunty  put  her  bib  on;  etc. 
The  next  day,  though  the  cold  continued  and  she  had  no  appetite, 
she  was  sunny  and  happy  all  day;  nor  was  there  any  recurrence  of 
the  peevishness  in  several  days  more  that  the  cold  lasted,  though 
she  visibly  suffered,  in  color  and  appetite,  from  its  long  continuance. 
In  the  twenty-eighth  month,  during  the  severest  cold  and  cough 
she  had  in  the  three  years,  I  note  one  day  that  she  is  not  seriously 
affected  in  spirits;  the  next  that  she  is  in  lively  spirits,  singing  and 
prattling  about  her  play;  the  next  that  she  was  ill-behaved  at 
dinner,  and  cried  when  reproved,  and  this  was  the  night  of  the  fit 
of  unreasonable  crying  described  below  (Sleep,  p.  289). 

There  were,  of  course,  continual  special  grievances  to  interrupt 
the  high  level  of  happiness.  In  the  first  year  there  were  bumps 
and  falls,  deprivations,  the  great  standing  affliction  of  being  dressed, 
etc.  But  these  troubles  were  few  in  number  compared  to  the 
pleasures;  and  it  is  quite  striking  to  see  with  what  ease  the  baby 
recovered  from  uncomfortable  feeling.  In  the  first  five  months, 
though  she  took  pleasure  in  objects  to  see  and  to  hold,  she  showed 
no  grief  at  their  withdrawal,  and  her  mother  would  unceremoniously 
take  a  thing  from  her  hand  and  lay  it  aside  in  a  way  that  would 
have  brought  an  inconsolable  wail  from  some  babies;  but  this  one 
would  merely  look  surprised  or  troubled  for  a  moment,  then  reach 
for  something  else.  In  the  twenty-third  week,  for  the  first  time,  she 
nearly  cried  several  times  at  having  things  taken  from  her,  or  being 
interfered  with  while  she  was  playing  with  them ;  and  in  the  next 
week  she  was  bitterly  grieved  at  such  an  incident,  and  cried  till  her 
eyes  were  red  :  these  were  the  sole  instances  in  the  first  six  months, 
so  far  as  my  notes  show.  In  the  next  half-year  I  find  three  or  four 
instances  every  month  of  crying,  sometimes  bitterly,  over  disappoint- 
ment, or  withdrawal  of  something  enjoyed.  The  first  week  of  the 
ninth  month  I  have  a  general  note  that  she  usually  cries  for  a  few 
seconds  —  till  diverted  —  when  anything  is  taken  away  that  she 
specially  likes.  As  a  rule,  she  gave  things  up  easily,  and  turned 
her  attention  to  something  else.  E.g.,  in  the  eleventh  month  (315th 
day)  I  had  the  camphor  beside  me  on  the  lawn  for  instant  applica- 
tion when  mosquitoes  attacked  her,  and  she  was  very  desirous  of 
17 


244  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

the  bottle,  and  crawled  round  and  round  me  after  it;  I  was  busy 
with  writing,  but  kept  an  eye  on  it,  and  removed  it  from  under  her 
grasp  just  as  she  reached  it,  again  and  again,  —  which  she  seemed 
to  regard  as  a  play,  and  took  in  perfect  good  nature.  When  she 
was  hurt,  bumped,  or  startled,  she  recovered  her  composure  very 
soon.  In  the  ninth  month,  her  locomotion  having  now  proceeded 
far  enough  to  cost  her  constant  bumps,  she  would  as  a  rule  look 
surprised  and  sober  for  a  few  seconds,  then  forget  the  matter;  a  few 
times,  there  was  lusty  crying.  In  the  tenth  month  she  paid  no 
attention  to  slight  bumps,  but  cried  vigorously  over  hard  ones.  In 
the  twelfth  she  hardly  passed  a  day  without  a  bump,  and  sometimes 
cried  hard  over  one,  but  was  easily  diverted. 

She  suffered  scarcely  at  all  from  timidity.  With  strangers  she 
was  as  a  rule  happy  and  sociable,  and  ready  to  respond  with  great 
gayety  to  advances  toward  a  frolic.  In  the  ninth  and  tenth  months 
she  suffered  a  little  from  timidity  toward  strangers,  and  she  had  a 
few  frights  from  severe  falls  and  other  causes;  but  on  the  whole  her 
absence  of  fear,  either  of  persons  or  things,  was  noticeable.  She 
was  not  easily  startled,  and  novel  sights  or  sounds  excited  a  pleasant 
interest  instead  of  fear. 

Throughout  the  second  and  third  years  the  increase  in  number 
and  force  of  desires  increased  not  only  the  child's  pleasures,  but  also 
her  disappointments.  There  was  also  an  increase  of  self-will,  and 
in  the  third  year,  especially  the  latter  part  of  it,  there  was  much 
disposition  to  resist  constraint.  There  was  therefore  less  easy  sur- 
render of  her  wishes,  and  notes  occur  from  time  to  time  of  crying 
over  refusals  or  deprivations;  hurts  too  were  sometimes  loudly 
lamented.  But  in  the  main  it  continued  true  that  the  child  threw 
off  disagreeable  feeling  readily  and  took  nothing  very  hard ;  accepted 
denial  reasonably;  would  let  a  tiling  be  taken  forcibly  from  her 
hand  with  little  protest,  and  was  easily  diverted.  She  was  always 
patient  and  cheerful  in  waiting  for  a  promised  pleasure,  dwelling  on 
the  pleasant  prospect,  not  the  delay.  "Ruth  has  had  one  peach," 
she  would  be  told,  "and  must  not  have  anymore  now;  another 
day."  —  "Yes:  Ruth  have  peach  another  day,"  the  child  would 
answer  contentedly,  and  trot  off  to  her  play  (twenty-second  month). 
The  day  after  her  severe  fall  and  concussion  of  the  brain,  she 
seemed  bright  and  natural,  only  a  little  sober,  and  was  unexpectedly 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  245 

little  troubled  by  her  bruises  (though  tired  toward  evening,  clinging 
to  her  mother  and  crying) ;  and  by  the  second  day  the  bruised  lip 
was  recovering  with  surprising  rapidity,  and  her  spirits  were  excel- 
lent. No  nervous  effects  were  ever  visible  from  the  accident. 
Anger  and  fear  (though,  especially  in  the  second  year,  there  were 
some  episodes  of  timidity  with  people)  troubled  her  very  little; 
and  specific  pleasures  far  outnumbered  specific  grievances  at  all 
times. 

A  mood  more  common  than  depression  or  iritability  was  one  of 
rough  boisterousness.  This  I  noted  but  once  in  the  first  year,  in 
the  twenty-eighth  week,  when  the  baby  took  up  a  sort  of  riotous 
behavior  and  became  very  noisy,  with  loud,  hoarse  shouting,  and 
rough  laughing,  "haw-haw,"  —  which  at  first  I  took  for  an  ex- 
pression of  discontent,  but  which  proved  after  a  day  or  two  to  be 
good-natured.  One  evening  in  this  week  she  sat  on  the  table  just 
before  bedtime,  shouting,  ho-hoing,  and  reaching  with  snatches  in 
every  direction,  as  if  she  could  not  keep  still,  —  spilling  over  with 
riotous  excitement,  till  her  gentle  grandmother  said,  "  Why,  you 
little  rowdy !"  In  the  next  week  this  noisy  behavior  disappeared, 
giving  place  to  a  more  tempered,  yet  more  joyous  merriment.  The 
first  instance  of  a  similar  wild  mood  that  I  record  in  the  second 
year  is  at  nineteen  months,  when  the  child  was  one  evening  in 
boiling-over  spirits,  especially  disposed  to  do  forbidden  things,  — 
twitching  the  table-cloth,  e.  g.,  to  the  risk  of  lamp  and  dishes  ;  she 
would  dive  with  an  air  of  triumphant  mischief  at  her  grandmother's 
glasses  and  cap,  and  jerk  them  off  before  a  hand  could  be  quick 
enough  to  stop  her.  Some  roguishness  often  at  this  time  —  twenti- 
eth month  —  entered  into  her  fits  of  wild  merriment,  usually  showed 
by  running  laughingly  away  from  us,  but  sometimes  by  seizing  at 
our  hair  (see  Muscular  Sensation,  p.  190).  In  the  twenty- first 
month  she  tried  repeatedly  to  see  how  far  she  could  go  safely  in 
roguish  naughtiness,  —  shouting  and  squealing  at  the  table,  throwing 
her  hat  out  of  the  buggy,  e.  g.  In  one  fit  of  wild  spirits,  at  twenty- 
one  months,  she  scrambled  defiantly  on  the  table  at  the  close  of  a 
meal,  and  seized  on  the  salts;  scampered  about  laughing,  impatient 
if  held  still  a  moment;  and  refused  to  kiss  any  of  us  good-night, 
laughing  and  romping  instead.     I  note  over  and  over  in  the  third 


246  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

year  that  her  naughtinesses  are  almost  entirely  pure  overflows  of 
rovvdyish  spirits,  —  such  as  throwing  her  plate  across  the  table, 
(twenty-fifth  month,)  banging  the  furniture  with  sticks,  gayly  assault- 
ing us  or  the  cat.  In  the  thirtieth  month  I  note  that  much  of  the 
time  the  child  seems  unable  to  contain  her  animal  spirits,  running 
over  with  desire  of  noise  and  motion,  yet  does  not  laugh  a  great 
deal,  and  is  rather  noisy  than  merry.  In  the  thirty-third  month, 
the  mood  seemed  still  more  clearly  differentiated  from  real  merri- 
ment: there  was  a  boisterous,  spilling-over  air  about  her,  that  kept 
her  kicking  this,  putting  her  foot  on  that,  in  a  naughty  way,  —  not 
investigating,  but  a  stupid  and  aimless  expenditure  of  nerve  surplus, 
which  seemed  to  unfit  her  for  occupation,  rather  than  to  show  a 
craving  for  it  as  an  outlet;  when  in  such  a  mood  she  would  not  be 
held  to  any  occupation,  but  was  restless  and  wilful.  She  had  a 
habit  at  the  time  of  moving  a  leg  about  or  teetering  on  her  feet 
almost  constantly,  without  any  reason  that  I  could  find.  It  seemed 
nervous  and  not  normal ;  and  I  have  seen  the  habit  in  an  exceedingly 
nervous  grown  person ;  but  the  child  seemed  in  perfectly  healthy 
condition. 

In  the  rough  gayety  there  was  little  natural  and  joyous  laugh- 
ter: indeed,  in  the  thirty-third  and  thirty-fourth  months  I  noticed 
the  infrequency  of  genuine  laughter,  and  wondered  if  it  was  because 
the  child's  association  was  so  entirely  with  grown  people;  she 
seemed  to  wish  to  laugh,  and  to  try  to  work  herself  up  to  it,  but  it 
was  forced.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  thirty-fifth  month  I  speak  of 
the  return  of  a  habit  of  joyous  laughter. 

From  what  sources,  physical  or  psychical,  these  moods  came,  I 
found  it  impossible  to  guess:  they  seemed  to  be  essentially  a  part 
of  the  child's  temperament,  and  in  a  modified  form,  they  persist  as 
she  grows  older.  Although  it  was  not  possible  always  to  draw  the 
line  between  the  outbreaks  of  boisterousness  and  those  of  pure 
merriment,  —  for  they  shaded  into  each  other,  —  they  were  never- 
theless different  things;  and  I  have  seen  no  reason  to  think  that 
there  is  any  gain  to  the  joyousness  of  childhood  from  indulging 
the  boisterous  tendency,  (often  seen  in  untrained  boys,)  nor  should 
it  be  confused  with  genuine  high  spirits  and  romping  glee. 

Up  to  the  56th  day,  so  far  as  my  notes  show,  the  general  mood 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  247 

of  the  baby  was  the  same  from  day  to  day,  (and  apparently  of  a 
neutral  character,)  brief  fluctuations  occurring  within  the  day.  On 
the  56th  day  I  find  the  first  mention  of  a  mood  as  characterizing 
the  whole  day:  the  baby  is  on  this  date  recorded  as  singularly 
bright  and  sunny  "all  day  long,"  smiling  at  every  one;  the  next 
day  was  a  sober  one;  and  after  this  I  find  over  and  over  notes 
of  moods  that  lasted  all  day,  or  day  after  day.  In  the  second 
and  third  years  I  find  sometimes  noted  mere  ebullitions  of  gayety, 
lasting  perhaps  an  hour  or  two;  sometimes  that  the  child  was 
spilling  over  with  jollity  and  activity  "all  day;"  and  sometimes  for 
weeks  at  a  time  her  uniform  physical  well-being  and  overflowing 
spirits,  her  racing,  capering,  squealing,  all  day  long  and  day  after 
day,  are  remarked  on;  or  perhaps  like  periods  of  soberer  mood. 
In  analyzing  these  notes,  it  becomes  evident  that  there  were  long 
waves  of  variation  in  mood,  within  which  occurred  the  shorter 
waves,  those  of  a  few  hours  or  a  few  days,  which  were  usually  quite 
clearly  referable  to  physical  condition.  The  longer  waves  were 
more  obscure  in  their  origin,  and  more  imperfectly  noted;  and  I 
have  tried  in  vain  to  find  any  consistent  process  of  development 
underlying  them.  The  following  periods  seem  fairly  clear  in  my 
record: — 

In  the  first  three  months  of  the  second  year  the  child's  pre- 
vailing mood  seems  to  have  been  rather  of  serious  happiness 
than  of  gayety,  her  increasing  powers  of  locomotion  and  under- 
standing opening  up  mental  interests  that  waked  all  her  curiosity 
and  absorbed  all  her  attention.  There  was  more  of  mere  merriment 
in  the  sixteenth  month,  but  somewhere  about  the  beginning  of  the 
seventeenth  a  vague  decline  in  spirits  was  perceived, — -a  loss  of  the 
expression  of  bright  and  winning  interest  from  her  face,  and  of  the 
habit  of  frequent  smiles  and  laughter;  the  expression  of  her  face 
became  prevailingly  dull.  This  phase  may  have  had  something  to 
do  with  dentition,  but  it  was  in  the  eighteenth  and  nineteenth 
months,  not  the  seventeenth  and  eighteenth,  that  dentition  was 
proceeding  most  rapidly.  A  phase  of  most  marked  and  demonstra- 
tive joyousness  began  about  the  last  week  of  the  twentieth  month, 
and  may  mark  the  close  ot  a  period  of  dentition  ;  or  complete  recov- 
ery from  colds  with  the  settled  warm  weather  (June);  or  a  stage  of 
especially  vigorous  circulation,  due  to  the  development  of  the  powers 


248  University  of  California.  [Vol  i. 

of  movement;  or  less  obvious  causes.  Her  even  nerves,  perfect 
physical  condition,  and  freedom  from  timidity,  are  expressly  noticed 
at  this  time. 

With  the  last  week  of  the  month,  and  until  near  the  end  of  the 
twenty-second,  appears  a  condition  of  being  "  not  quite  in  her 
usual  spirits,"  —  not  fretful,  but  less  busy  and  satisfied;  she  seemed 
often  bored  for  want  of  occupation,  restless,  desiring  occupation, 
but  unable  to  invent  much.  I  was  disposed  to  attribute  this 
mood,  which  recurred  in  the  twenty-fifth,  thirtieth,  and  thirty-third 
months,  to  psychic  causes,  thinking  the  time  had  come  when  her 
spontaneous  activities  were  not  enough,  and  she  needed  more 
guidance  in  her  play,  —  that  the  occupations  of  her  own  devising 
were  too  simple  and  monotonous,  and  she  desired  more  interesting 
ones,  supplied  by  us.  But  in  the  intervals  between  these  phases  of 
restlessness  I  find,  on  the  other  hand,  notes  of  increasing  ability  and 
disposition  to  occupy  herself.  It  is  evident  that  it  was  a  complex 
matter,  in  which  increasing  mental  demands  and  increasing  power 
of  self-occupation  kept  pace  but  irregularly;  and  general  physical 
mood  came  in  as  a  factor,  for  when  the  child  was  in  a  state  of  high 
well-being,  interest  was  easily  roused  by  slight  things  that  in  other 
moods  did  not  suffice.  Another  form  of  this  same  variation  in 
mood  as  indicated  by  interest,  was  that  at  times  the  child  was  more 
fickle  and  inconsecutive  in  occupation  than  at  other  times,  no  one 
thing  having  power  to  interest  her  long.  After  this  mood  in  the 
twenty-second  month,  with  gradually  increasing  gayety  through 
the  twenty-third,  the  twenty-fourth  seems  to  have  been  a  month  of 
exuberant  happiness. 

In  the  third  year,  there  was  a  good  deal  of  alternation,  periods 
of  restlessness  and  dependence  in  attention  alternating  with  others 
of  advancing  power  of  self-occupation,  and  periods  of  boisterous- 
ness  with  others  of  real  merriment,  lasting  from  a  few  days  to 
two  or  three  weeks;  but  my  notes  do  not  define  these  periods 
with  much  clearness,  nor  give  clue  to  their  causes.  The  twenty- 
seventh  month,  however,  was  one  of  lively  gayety,  while  from 
the  thirtieth  there  was  a  vague  diminution  of  spirits  and  increase 
of  the  rather  joyless  boisterousness  above  described.  During 
much  of  the  thirty-fourth  month,  the  child  seemed  quieter,  did 
not  jump  or   shout    much,  said    it    made    her   tired   to    run,  was 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  249 

willing  to  sit  in  our  laps,  or  even  sought  them,  though  she  seemed 
perfectly  well.  Before  the  end  of  the  thirty-fifth  month  the  bright 
and  spontaneous  gayety  reappeared,  and  so  to  the  end  of  the 
year.  Whimsical,  noisy,  and  rather  rowdy  fits  still  occurred, — 
freakish  ebullitions  at  table,  c.  g.,  —  but  on  the  whole  the  child  was 
charmingly  pleasant,  gentle,  and  joyous  in  these  closing  weeks  of 
the  year.1 

Some  other  peculiar  states,  apparently  due  to  a  more  than 
normal  intensifying  of  pleasurable  general  sensation  (besides  the 
psychic  elements)  are  mentioned  below  (p.  258)  in  connection  with 
their  effect  on  sleep.  Notice  especially  the  physical  effect  pervading 
the  baby's  whole  body  after  her  excitement  over  callers,  —  mak- 
ing her  alert  throughout,  wanting  to  jump  and  dance.  Other 
peculiar  states  attending  the  approach  and  disappearance  of  sleep, 
are  described  below  also. 

From  the  first,  the  bath  (340  C.)  seemed  to  have  a  marked 
effect  in  producing  a  heightened  sense  of  general  comfort,  and 
caused  an  expression  of  great  satisfaction,  even  in  the  earliest 
days;  muscular  activity,  and  also  the  tension,  or  tone,  of  the  mus- 
cles, was  at  the  same  time  increased.  These  effects  were  doubtless 
due  to  more  rapid  and  well-distributed  circulation.  In  the  fourth 
week,  when  in  the  bath,  the  baby  would  open  her  eyes  very  wide, 
and  hold  up  her  head  more  stiffly  than  at  other  times,  while  the 
asymmetry  of  her  eye  movements  and  the  aimless  waving  of  hands 
and  arms  increased.  In  the  second  and  third  months,  she  propped 
herself  strongly  with  her  knees  and  pushed  with  her  feet  in  the  bath. 
Asymmetric  eye  movements  continued  there  (fifteenth  week)  after 
they  had  disappeared  under  all  other  circumstances.  The  baby  did 
not  smile  nor  utter  sounds  in  the  bath,  but  had  an  air  of  almost 
eager,  though  quiet  pleasure,  — ■  eyes  stretched  wide  open,  rapid 
breathing,  head  held  up,  feet  pushing  (second  month);  in  the  sixth 
month  (the  bath  now  about  320  C.)  she  still  enjoyed  it  without 
hilarity,  but  with  much  quiet  pleasure;  in  the  seventh  she  began  to 
show  hilarity  in  the  bath,  splashing  with  her  arms;  in  the  eleventh 
she  would  stiffen  in  resistance  to  being  taken  out.     It  was  at  all 

1  See  a  rougher  analysis  of  these  phases,  which  passes  over  some  of  the 
briefer  alternations,  on  pp.  S9,  90. 


250  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

times,  and  remains  at  seven  years  old,  with  I  think  not  a  single 
exception  in  her  life,  a  most  exhilarating  pleasure;  was  invariably 
desired  (with  the  single  exception  that  she  once  in  the  nineteenth 
month  cried  and  resisted  at  being  brought  in  from  outdoors  for  her 
bath),  and  reluctantly  quitted.  In  the  twenty-fifth  month  she  once 
urged  her  wish  to  stay  in  forever:  "Ruth  won't  go  come  out  any 
more!"  Throughout  the  second  and  third  years,  she  prattled  and 
played  in  the  bath,  with  shouts,  squeals,  and  joyous  inarticulate 
sounds. 

The  effect  of  undressing  and  rubbing  with  oil,  a  daily  practice 
in  the  early  months,  I  have  spoken  of  above  (p.  182);  and  also 
have  mentioned  that  this  rubbing  sometimes  changed  the  baby's 
whole  mood,  —  as  on  the  88th  day,  when  she  had  been  fretful  all 
the  afternoon,  but  became  happy  and  smiling  after  being  undressed 
and  rubbed;  so  again  the  177th  day. 

The  child  was  much  in  the  open  air,  and  most  of  the  time  from 
the  fifth  to  the  tenth  month  the  weather  was  most  favorable.  She 
did  not  in  the  first  six  months  appear  especially  affected  in  general 
sensation  by  the  outdoor  air.  The  first  time  she  was  taken  out- 
doors, in  our  arms,  29th  day,  she  showed  no  especial  pleasure;  and 
though  from  the  next  time,  38th  day,  she  did  show  deep  pleasure 
when  taken  out,  this  seems  to  have  been  due  in  the  first  place  to 
the  motion  of  the  baby-carriage.  Late  in  the  seventh  month  (202d 
day)  her  grandmother  took  her  outdoors  (bareheaded  for  the  first 
time)  and  sat  quietly  down  on  the  veranda  with  her  saying  she 
wished  her  to  learn  to  love  the  outdoor  air  and  sunshine,  the  trees, 
and  flowers,  and  birds,  without  needing  the  baby-carriage  and  its 
motion.  The  baby  seemed  for  the  first  time  taken  possession  of  by 
that  joy  in  outdoors  that  afterward  was  so  strong.  She  sat  in  her 
grandmother's  lap  with  murmurs  of  delight;  and  thereafter  her  joy 
in  lying  on  a  blanket  and  rolling  freely  about  was  greatly  enhanced 
if  the  blanket  were  laid  on  veranda  or  lawn.  Wfthin  two  weeks, 
she  would  coax  to  be  taken  outdoors,  and  then  coax  till  she  was 
put  down  out  of  arms  and  left  to  the  enjoyment  of  her  own  perfect 
bliss.  In  the  following  months  my  notes  run:  "Great  joy  in  play- 
ing on  a  quilt  on  the  lawn,  —  sat  laughing  and  ejaculating  with 
pleasure;"  "Extreme  joy  in  being  allowed  to  sit  and  crawl  on  the 


Shinn.J  The  Development  of  a   Child.  251 

lawn,  —  incessant  cries  of  joy  and  laughter  as  she  looked  about,  at 
the  trees,  etc.,  up,  down,  and  around,  —  happiness  overflowing  for 
some  hour  and  a  half,  till  she  was  taken  away."  By  the  eleventh 
month,  to  go  outdoors  was  the  great  joy  of  her  life,  coaxed  for 
daily,  and  hailed  with  crows,  prattle,  laughter,  and  movements  of 
the  body;  in  driving,  the  baby  would  nestle  to  one  with  murmurs 
of  joy,  utter  small  shouts  and  joyous  syllables,  lean  to  look  about, 
clap  or  wave  her  hands,  smile  and  look  up  in  our  faces. 

Throughout  the  second  and  third  years  even  more  than  in  the 
first,  to  be  outdoors  was  the  great  condition  of  buoyant  states  of 
feeling.  Her  desire  to  go  out,  her  grief  in  refusal,  her  exceeding 
joy  in  going,  her  happiness  outdoors,  her  reluctance  to  come  in, 
are  the  subject  of  endless  notes.  Much  of  her  emotional  expression 
was  developed  in  connection  with  this  most  intense  and  constant  of 
her  desires  and  joys.  In  the  thirteenth  and  fourteenth  months,  e.g., 
she  would  coax  all  day  to  go  out,  going  to  the  window  and  pointing 
out,  to  the  door  and  prying  at  the  edge,  to  the  chest  where  her 
bonnet  was  kept  and  tugging  at  the  lid,  to  us  and  begging  with  all 
her  arts;  when  asked  if  she  wished  to  go,  she  would  nod  and  give 
cries  of  assent,  breaking  into  joyous  laughter,  and  when  she  saw 
movements  toward  getting  her  wraps  or  opening  the  door,  the 
laughter,  springing,  and  cries  would  increase;  when  brought  in,  she 
would  cry  and  beg  tragically,  "Go!  go!  go!"  (eighteenth  month). 
In  the  periods  of  dullest  spirits,  it  was  scarcely  ever  that  she  was 
not  happy  and  able  to  occupy  herself  outdoors;  while  in  the  hap- 
piest period  she  was  occasionally  a  little  fretful  if  kept  indoors. 
The  ebullitions  of  intensest  joy  and  activity  were  when  running 
about  outdoors:  e.  g.,  627th  day,  as  she  played  on  the  lawn,  it  did 
not  seem  possible  for  her  to  get  expression  enough  for  her  spirits,  — 
she  ran,  whirled  around,  shook  herself,  squealed,  laughed,  and  raced 
vigorously  this  way  and  that. 

In  this  enormous  effect  on  the  spirits,  the  greater  abundance 
and  novelty  of  objects  of  interest  did  not  (apart  from  the  dog  and 
other  animals)  seem  to  have  much  part,  for  she  did  not  seek  their 
resources  much :  she  sometimes  explored,  but  most  of  the  time  her 
outdoor  occupations  were  simpler  than  indoor  ones,  and  no  more 
novel,  —  she  sometimes  trotted  about  by  the  hour,  purposelessly 
enough;  she  often  picked  flowers;  she  dug  in  the  ground  a  great 


252  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

deal;  sometimes  she  simply  ran  along  the  garden  walks,  tossing 
her  arms  and  exulting.  She  repeatedly  begged  to  leave  a  novel 
house,  full  of  objects  of  interest,  to  "walk  outdoors,"  though  there 
was  no  more  than  a  board  walk  and  a  few  weeds  and  grasses  there 
to  interest  her.  Even  in  the  first  year,  I  did  not  think  that  the 
novel  sights  and  sounds  had  much  to  do  with  her  outdoor  joy,  for 
a  large  collection  of  interesting  things  to  see  and  hear,  in  a  room, 
never  produced  the  same  sort  of  unconcentrated,  incurious  joyous- 
ness.  Nor  could  the  wider  spaces  have  affected  her  much  in  the 
first  year,  since  distant  seeing  was  not  yet  acquired  (see  Sight, 
p.  20).  At  this  time,  I  thought  that  the  mere  physical  effect  of  the 
fresh  air,  together  with  the  brighter  light,  and  perhaps  the  moving 
and  playing  of  the  lights  in  the  leaves,  must  make  up  the  main  part 
of  the  pleasure.  In  the  second  and  third  years,  the  freedom  from 
walls  and  bounds,  the  larger  visible  spaces,  the  brighter  colors,  the 
movement  and  rustlings  in  the  trees,  the  outdoor  sounds,  seem  to 
have  made  up  a  joyous  medley  of  sense-impression  that  overflowed 
in  a  highly  diffused  exhilaration.  The  condition  seems  akin  to  the 
joy  of  birds;  and  there  seems  no  reason  to  doubt  that  it  is  an  early 
stage  of  true  "love  of  nature."  The  love  of  outdoor  freedom  is 
strongly  hereditary  in  this  child's  case:  her  remoter  ancestry,  as  I 
have  mentioned,  (p.  5)  was  mainly  of  seafaring  or  farming  folk;  and 
no  other  trait  is  common  to  so  many  of  her  kindred  in  the  gen- 
eration or  two  before  her.  Whether  it  is  any  more  marked  in  her 
than  in  other  children,  however,  I  do  not  know:  nothing  is  a  more 
invariable  observation  in  every  nursery  than  the  passion  for  out- 
doors. 

2.  Sleep,  and  Attendant  States  of  Sensation. 

[For  purposes  of  intelligible  record  it  is  impossible  to  separate 
the  subjects  named  above,  although  it  is  evident  that  Sleep,  a 
physiological  condition,  lies  quite  outside  the  subject  of  Sensation. 
It  is  attended,  both  in  coming  on  and  in  passing  away,  by  char- 
acteristic states  of  general  sensation,  and  also  by  one  distinct 
sensation,  —  that  of  sleepiness,  —  recognized  in  consciousness  as 
unlike  any  other,  and  somewhat  localized,  being  referred  vaguely 
to  the  head,  and  especially  to  the  eyes. 

Something  must  be  said,  also,  as  to  the  classification  of  Fatigue, 


Shinn.1  The  Development  of  a    Child.  253 

which  Preyer  makes  a  separate  head,  under  Organic  Sensations. 
The  word  fatigue  is  used  of  several  feelings,  which  fall  in  quite 
different  categories  of  sensation.  Muscular  fatigue  is  a  distinct 
sensation,  easily  localized  when  the  activity  that  produced  it  has 
been  local.  It  is  plainly  a  correlative  of  muscular  activity,  and  I 
have  already  entered  my  notes  concerning  it  under  the  head  of  Mus- 
cular Sensation.  There  are  also  the  somewhat  complex  feelings 
of  fatigue,  dermal,  muscular,  organic,  and  vascular,  which  I  have 
placed  above  under  the  head  of  Sensations  of  Position,  though  they 
might  perhaps  be  held  to  belong  to  general  sensation.  There  are 
also  states  of  completely  diffused  sensation  originating  in  excess  of 
nervous  action,  usually  of  the  special  senses,  with  some  accompany- 
ing cerebral  excitement;  and  these  I  class  in  the  present  chapter, 
the  more  as  their  relation  to  sleep  was  especially  noticeable.  Unlike 
the  others,  they  are  not  always,  or  usually,  feelings  of  lassitude,  but 
rather  of  increased  excitability,  and  the  word  fatigue  would  not 
be  used  in  naming  them  unless  the  excitability  had  passed  on 
into  the  irritable  condition  that  we  recognize  when  we  say  we  are 
"nervously  tired."  Fatigue  may  be,  again,  purely  cerebral,  as 
when  the  attention  is  wearied,  and  in  this  case  there  is  no  sensation 
at  all.  In  scientific  discussion  the  physiological  condition  that 
causes  all  these  feelings  is  almost  always  meant  when  the  word  is 
used,  not  the  feelings  themselves. 

The  word  being  used  thus  comprehensively,  and  the  condition 
of  fatigue  being  one  that  attends  (with  or  without  resulting  sensa- 
tion) all  activity,  I  have  made  no  separate  category  of  it,  but  have 
distributed  my  notes  of  the  different  types  of  fatigue  according  to 
what  seemed  their  simplest  relation  in  the  actual  occurrence  of  the 
incidents.] 

The  duration  of  sleep  in  the  early  months  was  quite  different 
in  the  case  of  my  niece  and  in  that  of  Preyer's  child.  Instead  of 
beginning  with  short  periods  of  sleep,  2  hours  in  the  first  month, 
and  gradually  increasing  up  to  6  or  8  hours  in  the  sixth  month, 
my  niece  had  her  long  periods  of  unbroken  sleep  in  the  first  two 
months,  and    shorter    ones    afterward.     From    the  first  week,  she 

1  This  does  not  ignore  the  fact  that,  as  several  psychologists  have  shown, 
there  is  also  a  faint  diffusion  of  fatigue  through  the  body  after  local  activity. 


254  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

often  slept  6  hours  at  a  stretch,  and  7  hours  was  not  unusual  in  the 
second  month;  once  (58th  day)  she  slept  8  hours  continuously;  and 
at  two  months  old  (63d  day)  she  once  slept  9^  hours.1  After  the 
first  two  months,  I  found  it  impossible  to  keep  close  account  of  the 
duration  of  sleep;  but  the  general  indication  of  my  notes  is  that 
the  periods  grew  shorter,  as  habit  established  itself  in  the  nursing. 
Until  the  second  quarter-year  the  baby  slept  a  good  deal  of  the 
daytime,  in  several  naps;  but  now  the  daytime  sleeps  decreased, 
and  two  naps  of  an  hour  and  a  half  each,  morning  and  afternoon, 
(with,  in  the  fourth  month,  one  or  two  brief  dozes  besides,)  became 
the  rule  until  near  the  end  of  the  first  year.  At  night  she  slept 
about  11  hours,  broken  by  two  intervals  of  nursing.  Now  and 
then  a  nap  in  the  daytime  extended  to  2^/2  or  3  hours,  and  now  and 
then  she  refused  one  nap  altogether.  Not  infrequently  she  slept 
past  the  midnight  nursing,  making  seven  or  eight  hours  of  unbroken 
sleep;  once,  in  the  twelfth  month,  although  she  waked  for  milk  at 
midnight,  she  slept  until  9  A.  M.,  three  hours  past  her  usual  time 
of  waking. 

By  the  end  of  the  first  year  it  became  difficult  to  coax  two  naps 
in  the  daytime,  and  in  the  thirteenth  month  one  became  the  rule. 
Even  when  she  seemed  very  sleepy,  it  would  prove  impossible 
actually  to  get  her  to  sleep.  The  naps  seem  to  have  become 
shorter  and  lighter,  also,  though  my  notes  are  insufficient  on  this 
point;  I  have  one  note  (nineteenth  month)  of  a  nap  of  four  hours 
There  were  a  good  many  days  when  the  baby  refused  to  sleep  at 
all;  I  find  one  note  of  four  days  in  succession  without  a  daytime 
nap  (twenty-first  month). 

In  the  third  year,  the  daytime  nap  grew  harder  and  harder  to 

'The  first  of  these  long  sleeps  (37th  day)  illustrated  the  decrease  of  depth 
of  sleep  in  its  latter  hours.  She  fell  asleep  at  5  p.  m.,  though  it  was  then 
nursing-time,  and  slept  deeply  till  9  o'clock.  On  later  occasions  of  prolonged 
sleep  the  mother,  finding  that  no  harm  came  from  postponement  of  nourish- 
ment, let  the  baby  sleep  as  long  as  she  would;  but  this  first  time  she  waked  her 
when  the  interval  since  nursing  had  reached  six  hours.  The  baby  nursed  and 
dropped  at  once  into  deep  sleep  again,  and  slept  till  midnight,  when  she  was 
again  fed  (my  note  does  not  say  whether  she  waked  voluntarily)  and  dropped 
again  to  sleep;  but  though  earlier  in  the  night  she  had  slept  so  soundly  that 
hunger  could  not  wake  her  even  at  three  times  the  ordinary  interval  since  she 
had  been  fed.  she  now,  though  fed  and  comfortable,  slept  lightly,  and  waked 
three  or  four  times  before  morning. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  255 

induce,  and  more  irregular;  at  this  time,  too,  her  mother  was 
obliged  to  be  often  absent  from  her,  which  increased  the  difficulty; 
and  after  the  twenty-sixth  month,  the  regular  nap  was  given  up. 
She  not  infrequently  fell  asleep,  however,  when  driving,  or  travel- 
ing on  the  cars,  and  several  times  when  ailing  took  short  naps. 
Meanwhile,  in  both  the  second  and  third  years,  the  sleep  at  night 
was  usually  about  10  hours  (8  p.  m.  to  6  A.  m.),  sometimes  broken 
once  or  twice.  In  a  few  cases  in  the  third  year  this  was  prolonged 
to  11,  and  once  (twenty-seventh  month)  to  13  hours. 

Opportunities  to  observe  the  effect  of  peripheral  stimulus  during 
sleep  were  small.  As  far  as  sounds  were  concerned,  the  sensibility 
was  slight  in  the  early  weeks,  and  it  was  not  till  the  last  week  of  the 
fourth  month  that  voices  and  movements  about  the  baby  were  apt 
to  make  her  restless  in  sleep.  The  rustling  of  paper,  however, 
(the  nurse  told  me,)  repeatedly  made  her  start  and  cry  as  early  as 
the  3d  or  4th  day,  both  when  asleep  and  when  awake;  and  on  the 
6th  day  I  heard  her  cry  out  in  her  sleep  at  the  sharp  tearing  of 
paper.  I  noted  once  in  the  twelfth  month  (243d  day)  that  she 
waked  every  time  her  father  rustled  a  paper. 

In  the  following  months  her  grandmother  thought  her  rather 
easy  to  waken,  as  compared  with  other  babies:  still,  she  was  not 
wakened  in  the  evening  by  the  talking  and  reading  aloud  that 
regularly  took  place  in  an  adjoining  room,  separated  by  a  portiere 
only  from  the  one  in  which  she  slept;  and  several  times  she  slept  on 
the  cars  without  being  perceptibly  disturbed  by  the  stopping  and 
starting  at  stations,  the  coming  and  going  of  people,  nor  even  the 
brakeman's  calling  of  stations  and  slamming  of  doors,  unless  she 
were  near  the  end  of  the  car.1  Like  most  babies  she  could  be  lifted, 
carried,  and  laid  down,  without  waking,  if  it  was  carefully  done:  her 
sleep  in  these  earliest  years,  however,  never  approached  the  insensi- 

1  Whether  in  response  to  sensations,  as  weariness  of  one  position,  weight  of 
bedclothes,  etc.,  or  from  central  causes,  she  has  since  the  second  year  (like 
most  children)  shown  a  great  deal  of  muscular  activity  in  sleep.  In  the  last 
week  of  the  second  year,  I  slept  with  her,  and  noticed  the  remarkable  restless- 
ness with  which  she  rolled  about  in  all  imaginable  positions,  e.g.,  across  my 
breast  and  neck,  or  with  her  face  lying  on  my  ear,  etc.  All  this  was  habitual, 
her  mother  assured  me.  When  she  slept  beside  me  in  the  third  year,  it  was 
not  quite  so  marked. 


256  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

bility  to  external  stimulus  that  characterizes  it  now,  at  seven  years 
old,  when  the  child  can  be  taken  out  of  one  bed  and  tumbled  into 
another  quite  heedlessly  without  disturbing  her,  and  has  to  be 
spoken  to  over  and  over  and  handled  rather  roughly  if  it  is  necessary 
to  rouse  her  out  of  a  deep  sleep.  By  the  middle  of  the  third  year, 
(and  ever  since,)  flea  and  mosquito  bites  disturbed  sleep  badly  (see 
Dermal  Sensations,  p.  150). 

In  light  drowsing  the  condition  of  sensibility  was  quite  differ- 
ent. Once  in  the  eleventh  month  (309th  day)  I  note  that  though 
sleepy  she  could  not  get  to  sleep  on  the  cars,  rousing  at  each 
station  from  the  doze  into  which  she  was  sinking,  disturbed  by 
the  change  of  motion.  If  she  went  to  sleep  in  one's  arms  it 
was  necessary  to  wait  till  sleep  became  deep  before  laying  her 
down.  There  may  have  been  in  light  half-sleep  an  even  heightened 
sensibility  to  the  changes  of  motion,  as  there  was  to  sound 
in  the  incident  of  her  being  startled  at  the  rattling  of  the  tin 
bath-tub  handles  on  the  37th  day  (see  Hearing,  p.  108);  she  was 
never  thus  startled  by  any  sound  when  awake.  For  discriminating 
sensibility,  her  senses  were  doubtless  dulled  in  this  condition  of 
half-sleep,  as  older  people's  are.  Thus  in  the  last  week  of  the 
second  year  she  came  into  her  mother's  bed  before  light,  and  half- 
asleep,  rolled  over  to  me  as  I  occupied  her  mother's  usual  place, 
and  though  she  touched  my  face  and  hair  with  her  hands,  did  not 
discover  till  daylight  that  it  was  not  her  mother.  See  also  instances 
below  of  clearness  or  confusion  of  senses  just  after  waking.  Refer- 
ences will  be  noticed  below,  in  other  connections,  to  her  apparent 
consciousness  sometimes,  even  when  half  asleep,  of  our  move- 
ment, —  clinging  tighter,  e.g.,  if  I  moved,  when  she  had  gone  to 
sleep  clinging  to  me,  —  automatic  probably,  under  continuance  of 
the  suggestion  given  by  her  waking  fear  that  I  would  leave  her. 

Organic  sensations  —  or  conditions  —  affected  the  duration 
and  depth  of  sleep  considerably,  as  well  as  the  ease  with  which  it 
came  on.  The  satisfaction  of  hunger  favored  sleep,  and  it  was 
a  daily  practice  to  go  to  sleep  at  the  breast:  yet,  as  has  been 
mentioned  above  (Hunger,  p.  215)  hunger  did  not  always  suf- 
fice to  prevent  sleep,  nor  to  break  it.  Sleep  was  apt  to  be 
deeper  and  longer   in  the  open  air,  and  when  the  weather  per- 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  257 

mitted  the  baby  often  had  her  naps  outdoors.  She  was  far  more 
easily  put  to  sleep  in  the  open  air  all  through  the  first  and  second 
years ;  as  this  was  always  in  her  baby-carriage  or  on  a  drive,  the 
motion  may  have  been  the  chief  reason,  but  pushing  her  about  in 
the  baby-carriage  inside  the  house  did  not  have  the  same  effect. 
At  the  end  of  the  ninth  month  (271st  day)  she  slept  peacefully  an 
hour  and  a  half  on  the  deck  of  a  yacht,  amid  the  talk  and  laughter 
of  a  large  party  and  the  noises  of  the  rigging,  but  when  below, 
roused  at  any  noise.  There  seemed  also  to  be  after-effect  on  her 
sleep  from  fresh  air  :  after  returning  from  a  drive  or  being  outdoors 
a  long  time,  she  would  sleep  more  readily  and  soundly.  On  the 
141st  day  she  fretted  and  cried  unusually  over  going  to  sleep,  with- 
out any  cause  that  I  could  conjecture  except  that  she  had  missed 
her  daily  outing. 

Difficulty  in  breathing,  from  a  cold,  waked  her  over  and  over 
(three  or  four  days  in  the  last  week  of  the  eighth  month)  even  when 
she  was  tired  and  sleepy. 

I  find  but  the  slightest  indication  of  disturbance  of  sleep  from 
digestive  conditions,  or  general  derangements  connected  with  teeth- 
ing, —  once  in  the  sixth  month,  two  or  three  fretful  days,  with 
unusual  difficulty  in  going  to  sleep,  while  she  had  a  cold,  and  two 
teeth  nearly  through;  in  the  tenth  month,  several  nights  of  broken 
sleep,  with  bad  appetite  in  the  daytime,  which  may  have  been  due 
to  teething,  or  to  diet ;  and  a  short  period  in  the  eighteenth  month 
(p.  233,  above)  when  disturbance  of  sleep  seemed  due  to  a  habit  of 
eating  between  meals,  and  ceased  as  soon  as  the  practice  was 
stopped.     For  sleepiness  after  nausea,  see  p.  235,  above. 

I  gave  a  good  deal  of  attention  to  the  after-effect  upon  sleep  of 
the  activities  of  the  day.  I  never  could  see  that  muscular  activity, 
however  considerable,  produced  any  effect  on  sleep.  Neither  did 
mental  exertion,  if  unaccompanied  by  excitement.  A  day  of  the 
most  eager  and  interested  investigation  of  objects,  e.  g.,  so  long  as 
it  was  quite  self-inspired,  never  seemed  to  leave  any  sort  of  nervous 
disturbance  behind,  nor  to  have  to  be  made  up  for  by  increased 
sleep.  The  effect  of  social  excitement,  or  rapidly  changing  mental 
occupation  (more  rapidly  than  the  natural  limits  of  her  attention 
called  for)  was  quite  different,  and  very  noticeable.     Here  appeared 


2 5^>  University  oj    California.  [Vol.  i. 

the  effects  of  nervous  over-activity  referred  to  above  (p.  253).  The 
first  instance  was  at  the  beginning  of  the  second  month,  when  callers 
came  in  to  congratulate  the  mother  and  see  the  baby.  On  the  first 
three  days  of  the  month  (32d  —  34th  days)  she  saw  a  number  of 
people,  and  as  she  now  noticed  faces  and  voices  her  attention  was 
a  good  deal  taxed.  She  seemed  restless  afterward  and  slept  less 
than  usual,  waking  more  easily. 

At  about  four  months  old  she  had  several  exciting  days,  all 
marked  by  happy  interest  during  the  excitement,  but  broken  rest 
afterward.  On  the  121st  day,  her  mother's  sister  was  married  from 
the  house,  and  what  with  the  arrival  and  dressing  of  bride  and 
maids,  the  trip  to  church,  where  the  baby  was  seized  and  passed 
about  among  kinsfolk  she  had  never  seen,  and  the  return  of  the 
bridal  party  for  lunch,  her  habits  were  entirely  upset.  She  took  all 
this  with  placid  cheerfulness,  although  she  had  lost  her  morn- 
ing nap  and  had  been  subjected  to  an  extra  dressing;  and  after  the 
company  was  gone  and  the  house  quiet,  and  we  looked  for  fatigue, 
she  showed  only  a  sort  of  pleased  excitement,  and  perhaps  a 
heightened  receptivity  to  sense  impressions  (see  Music,  p.  115, 
incident  of  the  121st  day);  but  she  could  not  be  got  into  a  sleep 
that  would  last  more  than  a  few  minutes,  all  the  afternoon  and 
evening;  and  during  the  night  she  waked  repeatedly,  always  happy 
and  wanting  to  play.  The  1 30th  day  the  baby  was  with  her  mother 
while  she  made  and  received  several  calls;  and  afterward  showed 
again  a  pleasant  excitement,  — -  alert  through  her  whole  body, 
wanting  to  jump,  and  dance,  and  "feel  her  feet."  She  was  coaxed 
to  sleep  with  difficulty,  and  waked  more  than  usual  in  the  night. 
The  136th  day  a  reception  was  given  to  the  bridal  couple:  while 
the  house  was  made  ready,  the  baby  was  perfectly  content  to  be 
neglected,  as  long  as  she  could  look  on  at  the  bustle;  and  while  the 
rooms  were  occupied  with  company,  she  was  happy  and  interested, 
with  a  smile  for  almost  every  one  who  spoke  to  her.  Her  mother 
took  her  away  upstairs  and  put  her  to  sleep,  but  she  waked  in  five 
minutes,  eager  to  go  on  with  the  fun,  and  would  not  sleep  again. 
This  time  she  became  a  little  fretful  by  evening,  but  I  have  no 
record  of  restlessness  at  night. 

So  again  in  the  eighth  and  thirteenth  months  (220th,  221st, 
3S8th  days)  I  find  instances  of  happy  excitement  during  the  day, 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  259 

followed  by  restlessness,  broken  sleep,  and  unusual  trouble  and 
timidity  on  waking.  In  the  tenth  month  her  father  used  to  take 
her  in  his  arms  and  run  about  with  her  in  the  garden,  calling  her 
attention  to  many  things.  This  caused  excited  joy,  followed  by  fits 
of  unusual  peevishness,  with  tears  and  clamorous  desire  for  new 
excitement;  I  have  not  noted,  however,  the  effect  upon  sleep.  As 
a  rule,  after  the  middle  of  the  second  year,  trips  were  taken  and  a 
great  deal  of  sight-seeing  for  so  young  a  child  experienced,  with- 
out noticeable  excitement,  or  after-effect  of  restlessness ;  but  there 
were  exceptions  to  this.  Even  in  the  first  year  there  were  times 
when  an  exciting  day  seemed  to  have  no  after-effect  on  sleep;  and 
it  was  noticeable  that  excitement  from  discomfort  and  distress 
passed  away  more  readily,  without  affecting  sleep,  than  excitement 
from  amusement.  There  were  times,  on  the  other  hand,  when  after 
the  most  unexciting  days  the  baby  fretted  and  would  not  sleep,  or 
wished  to  play  instead  of  sleeping,  and  would  wake  laughing  in  a 
few  minutes  after  she  had  been  coaxed  to  sleep. 

In  a  single  case  (ninth  month,  266th  day)  sleep  may  have  been 
disturbed  by  nervousness  following  on  agitation  from  pain  and  dis- 
comfort: the  baby  had  fallen  and  skinned  her  lip,  and  afterward  fell 
again  on  her  face;  had  cried  in  vain  to  be  taken  outdoors  (her  one 
great  comfort  for  all  ills,  refused  her  this  time  on  account  of  the 
extreme  heat  of  the  weather);  had  been  at  last  consoled  by  per- 
mission to  pull  books  off  some  low  shelves,  and  then  had  been 
interrupted  suddenly  in  this  charming  occupation  and  carried  off  to 
be  put  to  sleep.  This  succession  of  grievances  put  sleep  to  flight: 
she  cried  and  resisted  for  an  hour,  whenever  her  mother  tried  to 
lay  her  down,  and  got  only  such  sleep  as  she  could,  dozing  in  her 
mother's  arms.  It  is  likely  that  the  effect  on  sleep  in  this  case  was 
due  less  to  the  previous  excitement  from  the  fall,  than  to  the 
abrupt  disturbance  of  a  pleasant  occupation:  as  early  as  the  fifth 
month  I  note  interruption  of  occupation  as  a  cause  of  difficulty 
in  getting  the  baby  to  sleep;  and  from  the  ninth  month  on  to  the 
middle  of  the  second  year  instances  occur  over  and  over  showing 
that  when  she  was  interested  in  something  and  was  taken  suddenly 
from  it  to  be  put  to  sleep,  the  disappointment,  and  probably  also 
some  nervous  shock  in  the  sudden  wrench  of  attention  (such  as  we 
ourselves  experience  in  interruptions,  sometimes  with  a  feeling  of 
18 


260  University  of  California.  [Vol  i. 

almost  physical  distress)  made  it  very  hard  to  quiet  her  and  get 
her  to  sleep.     See  also  pp.  274,  275,  below. 

In  the  second  six  months  the  fretfulness  coming  from  lack  of 
sleep  seemed  of  itself,  sometimes,  to  prevent  sleep,  I  have  notes 
of  unusual  sleepiness  after  a  nap  had  been  missed:  but  on  the  other 
hand,  in  the  last  week  of  the  ninth  month  (239th  day),  when  the 
baby  had  lost'  her  afternoon  nap  on  account  of  a  cold,  she  was  got 
to  sleep  with  much  lulling  at  bedtime,  waked  in  fifteen  minutes, 
and  cried  for  an  hour  at  intervals,  while  her  mother  tried  to  lull 
her;  the  cry  grew  very  nervous  toward  the  end,  and  the  baby  shed 
tears  till  her  eyes  were  red.  I  thought  her  too  fretted  to  get  to 
sleep;  and  after  her  father  came  home  and  played  a  while  with  her, 
thus  relaxing  the  nervous  tension,  she  dropped  easily  to  sleep. 
Her  mother  thought  that  as  a  rule  in  this  month  she  went  to  sleep 
more  easily  if  she  was  taken  before  she  grew  fretful  with  sleepiness. 
When  a  nap  had  been  broken,  and  the  baby  had  become  fretful 
over  the  loss  of  sleep,  it  was  harder  to  lull  her  again  than  if  she 
had  not  been  so  sleepy;  and  sometimes  she  would  not  sleep  again, 
though  visibly  sleepy  the  rest  of  the  day.  I  note  one  instance, 
however,  (361st  day,)  in  which  she  was  fretful  with  sleep,  crying 
easily,  and  was  very  willing  to  go  to  sleep,  gladly  assenting  to  the 
suggestion  of  "  bylow." 

Though,  as  I  have  said,  mental  activity  without  excitement  had 
no  after-effect  on  sleep,  interest  easily  dispelled  sleepiness  for  the 
time  being.  This  first  appeared  in  the  second  month,  47th  day, 
when  the  baby  had  been  waked  from  a  nap,  and  was  fretting 
persistently  with  sleepiness;  as  her  mother  could  not  take  her  at 
once  to  put  to  sleep,  I  carried  her  to  the  piano  and  began  striking 
chords  to  divert  her;  she  became  silent  and  absorbed  immediately, 
and  did  not  renew  her  crying  till  her  mother  came  and  took  her 
away.  In  the  fifth  month  (133d  day)  she  had  rubbed  her  eyes  and 
seemed  sleepy  in  my  arms,  so  I  gave  her  to  her  mother  to  be  put 
to  sleep;  but  she  was  no  sooner  settled  than  she  began  turning  her 
head  to  look  at  me,  and  would  not  settle  to  sleep  till  I  was  out  of 
sight,  and  she  had  looked  for  me  for  some  minutes.  The  136th 
day,  when  undressed  while  fretting  with  sleepiness,  she  stopped 
fretting  at  sight  of  her  toes,  and  reached  for  them.  Up  to  the  end 
of  the  fifth  month  I  could  read  in  a  low  voice  to  the  mother  while 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  2  or. 

she  nursed  the  baby  and  lulled  her  to  sleep;  but  after  that  gave  it 
up,  as  my  presence  and  voice  were  apt  to  interest  her  too  much 
(see  Hearing,  p.  m).  Late  in  the  sixth  month,  when  she  was 
fretful  with  sleepiness,  and  for  some  reason  was  not  to  be  put  at 
once  to  sleep,  I  took  her  into  the  kitchen,  where  she  became  deeply 
and  happily  absorbed  in  watching  the  dish-washing;  but  when  she 
was  taken  away  she  cried  hard,  showing  that  she  was  still  sleepy, 
but  had  been  potently  interested.  The  171st  day  she  would  not 
sleep  on  a  train,  but  sat  staring  with  wide  eyes.  At  a  year  old,  she 
would  not  go  to  sleep,  though  very  sleepy,  in  a  large  empty  room, 
where  the  voice  that  was  lulling  her  echoed  from  the  walls  in  a  way 
that  interested  her  deeply.  In  the  twentieth  month,  though  she 
had  been  twice  interrupted  in  a  nap,  she  would  not  go  to  sleep  on 
a  ferry-boat,  but  insisted  on  trotting  round  and  round,  exploring 
with  great  interest.  In  the  second  and  third  years,  when  I  some- 
times put  her  to  sleep  (as  related  below)  with  stories,  I  had  to  take 
care  to  make  them  uninteresting,  —  long,  droning  narrations. 

It  is  worth  notice  that  in  babies  the  sensation  of  sleepiness 
seems  so  often  to  be  a  highly  uncomfortable  and  irritating  one, 
instead  of  a  mere  languor,  as  with  us.1  After  the  middle  of  the 
second  month,  instead  of  simply  becoming  quiet  and  drowsy,  the 
baby  would  often  show  want  of  sleep  by  fretfulness.  (I  noticed 
on  the  47th  day  that  just  as  in  the  case  of  hunger  the  pleasant 

1  The  question  has  been  raised  whether  it  is  in  fact  sleepiness,  or  the  preven- 
tion of  sleep  after  sleepiness  has  arrived,  that  causes  the  irritation.  We  ourselves 
find  the  feeling  of  resisted  sleep  (as  in  trying  to  keep  awake  during  a  dull  address) 
most  unpleasant;  but  the  discomfort  is  not  of  the  nature  of  irritability.  On  the 
other  hand,  when  we  have  lost  sleep,  yet  fail  to  experience  a  normal  sleepiness, 
we  are  apt  to  feel  instead  a  marked  irritability.  It  is  probable  that  with  both 
infant  and  adult  the  fretful  condition  is  not  properly  sleepiness  at  all,  but  a 
form  of  fatigue,  which  appears  much  more  frequently  in  infants  than  in  adults, 
unless  the  latter  suffer  from  insomnia.  This  may  be  largely  because  the  habit 
of  regular  hours  of  sleep,  at  which  the  drowsy  feeling  will  normally  appear, 
is  not  yet  established  for  the  infant.  It  is  usually  in  the  first  year  that  the 
fretful  ■'sleepiness  "  is  most  apparent;  and  in  my  niece's  case  the  second  year 
showed  a  time  habit  in  sleep  already  of  some  importance:  at  the  beginning  of 
the  17th  month,  an  attempt  to  put  her  to  sleep  before  the  usu^l  time  apparently 
made  her  very  wakeful,  and  the  last  night  of  the  21st  month,  though  she 
seemed  so  tired  and  sleepy  that  she  was  put  to  bed  two  hours  before  her  usual 
time,  she  did  not  go  to  sleep  for  an  hour  and  a  half. 


262  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

mood  never  passed  gradually  into  this  fretful  sleepiness,  but  always 
suddenly.)  I  have  notes  in  every  month  of  the  first  year,  after  the 
first,  of  fretting  with  sleepiness.  Sensibility  to  disagreeables  was 
heightened:  in  the  fourth  month,  to  be  undressed  while  sleepy 
was  a  great  grievance,  sometimes  causing  wails  and  tears,  though 
oftener  merely  whimpering;  I  note  instances  of  the  same  behavior 
in  the  sixth  month,  and  throughout  the  second  and  third  years 
she  was  far  more  ready  to  cry  if  anything  was  taken  from  her,  or 
if  she  was  interrupted  in  any  pleasure,  or  denied  some  whim,  when 
she  was  somewhat  sleepy.  In  the  open  air,  when  wheeled  or  driven 
about,  sleep  almost  always  came  on  without  any  such  fretfulness. 
The  power  of  attention,  memory,  etc.,  was  lowered:  at  fifteen 
months,  e.g.,  she  became  troubled  when  pressed  to  give  a  word 
while  sleepy,  though  she  knew  it  well  and  was  usually  very  willing 
to  give  it. 

Rubbing  eyes  as  a  sign  of  sleepiness  is  first  noted  in  the  fifth 
month,  133d  day:  it  was  already  a  habit. 

It  seems  somewhat  anomalous  that  a  condition  so  natural  and 
necessary  as  sleep  should  have  to  be  artificially  induced  in  the 
infant;  and  I  find  that  some  standard  medical  authorities  on  the 
care  of  infants  say  unhesitatingly  that  no  baby  need  be  lulled  to 
sleep,  but  that  a  darkened  room,  quiet,  and  comfortable  posi- 
tion, at  the  proper  time,  will  always  bring  sleep,  if  no  other  influ- 
ences have  ever  been  used.  Other  standard  authorities  admit,  if 
a  little  reluctantly,  the  necessity  of  lulling  in  many  cases.  Sooth- 
ing the  baby  to  sleep  with  songs  and  movements  seems  to  have 
been  the  custom  of  mothers  of  all  times  and  races,  and  it  is  hardly 
likely  that  a  practice  so  arduous  would  have  been  so  uniformly 
maintained  had  it  been  found  superfluous.  Even  monkey  mothers 
lull  their  babies  in  their  arms,  the  cat  licks  her  kittens  to  sleep,  and 
the  hen  croons  to  the  chicks  under  her  wings.  It  is  usually  neces- 
sary for  grown  people  to  compose  themselves  to  sleep  for  some  time 
by  deliberate  effort,  —  an  effort  that  must  necessarily  be  made  in 
behalf  of  a  young  infant  by  some  one  else.  There  are  certainly 
normal  differences  between  children  in  the  ease  with  which  they 
fall  asleep.  Many  children  will  drop  asleep  amid  their  toys  as  they 
play  about  the  floor,  in  their  high-chairs  at  table,  with  food  in  their 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  263 

hands,  or  as  they  sit  resting  in  some  one's  lap,  listening  to  stories, 
etc.  My  niece  was  never  but  three  times  in  her  life  thus  overtaken 
by  sleep:  and  at  seven  years  old,  when  many  active  and  healthy 
children  fall  asleep  "the  minute  their  heads  touch  the  pillow,"  as  the 
saying  is,  she  lies  for  some  time,  often  half  an  hour,  striving  to 
sleep,  and  composing  herself  with  all  the  songs  and  poems  in  her 
repertoire. 

It  is  frequently  urged  that  even  if  the  baby  will  not  go  to  sleep 
peacefully  when  laid  down  by  himself,  he  will  cry  himself  to  sleep 
if  left  alone,  and  in  time  will  learn  to  fall  asleep  without  crying. 
The  wisdom  of  leaving  a  baby  to  cry  himself  to  sleep  is  not  a 
medical  question,  but  a  psychological  one,  and  I  cannot  see  that 
here  the  opinion  of  doctors  has  the  value  of  that  of  competent 
mothers.  Some  careful  mothers  and  nurses  report  most  favorably 
of  the  practice.  Others  oppose  it  strongly,  saying  that  a  child  left 
to  cry  himself  to  sleep  may,  instead  of  bringing  about  a  gentle 
fatigue,  grow  more  and  more  excited,  and  cry  with  hysterical  vio- 
lence that  might  in  some  cases  cause  rupture,  and  certainly  drives 
away  sleep ;  that  when  sleep  does  come  after  such  distress  and 
excitement  it  is  less  restful  than  when  the  child  was  soothed  as 
tranquilly  as  possible;  and  that  the  general  effect  on  the  nervous 
system  is  bad.  Old-fashioned  mothers  say,  also,  that  the  approach 
of  sleep  is  accompanied  by  exceptional  conditions  of  susceptibility, 
which  the  mother  should  not  fail  to  take  advantage  of  for  the 
emotional  development  of  the  child ;  that  it  is  in  the  mother's 
lullaby  and  the  association  of  her  soothing  and  protecting  presence, 
from  the  earliest  dawn  of  consciousness,  with  this  time  of  oncoming 
sleep,  that  some  of  the  deepest  roots  are  struck  of  the  emotional 
relation  between  mother  and  child,  which  give  it  the  almost  mys- 
tical character  it  assumes  in  memory  in  after  life;  that  as  the  child 
grows  older,  the  bedtime  hour  is  pre-eminently  the  time  she  should 
hold  sacred  to  him,  in  which  she  will  find  his  heart  most  open  to 
all  tender  influence,  and  his  confidence  most  completely  hers;  that 
the  time  and  strength  demanded  by  this  ministry  should  no  more 
be  grudged  by  the  devoted  mother  than  she  should  grudge  nursing 
her  baby.  Probably  it  is  a  matter  in  which  extremes  are  to  be 
avoided:  every  one  has  seen  cases  in  which  babies  have  become 
spoiled  and   tyrannical  about  going  to  sleep,  and  cases  in  which 


264  University  of  California.  Vol.  i. 

mothers  have  inflicted  much  distress  on  themselves  and  the  baby 
in  compelling  the  little  lonely,  excited  creature  to  cry  itself  to  sleep 
for  the  sake  of  good  discipline.  Practically,  too,  the  question  of 
putting  children  to  bed  is  apt  to  be  complicated  by  rivalry  between 
their  claims  and  the  husband's  for  the  wife's  society  in  the  evening. 

As  to  the  farther  question  of  methods  of  lulling,  there  has  been 
general  medical  objection  to  the  cradle.  It  is  said  that  the  sleep 
induced  by  motion  is  not  natural  sleep,  but  of  a  hypnotic  nature. 
¥\oss(Das  Kleine  Kind,  pp.  65-70)  sums  up  the  opinion  of  different  Ger- 
man authorities,  and  concludes  that  the  real  objection  is  not  to  gentle 
rocking  into  sleep,  —  which  is,  he  says,  essentially  nature's  method, 
as  the  infant  is  carried  about  on  the  mother's  back,  or  lulled  in  her 
arms  by  a  gentle  swaying  of  her  body,  —  but  to  rough  and  jolting 
motions,  and  to  continuing  the  rocking  after  the  child  is  asleep,  as 
the  cradle  tempts  one  to  do.  I  have  pointed  out  above  that  the 
young  child  is  certainly  less  susceptible  to  disagreeable  sensations 
from  jarring  or  from  dizziness  than  the  adult,  and  it  is  possible  that 
the  solicitude  as  to  the  effects  of  rocking  infers  too  much  from  adult 
conditions.  Even  in  the  case  of  adults,  there  is  no  evidence  that 
sailors  suffer  any  harm  from  sleeping  amid  the  motions  of  the  ship, 
and  people  go  yachting  expressly  for  the  sake  of  the  refreshing 
sleep  which  they  claim  the  motion  brings  them.  Birds,  whose 
development  of  the  ear-canals  must  make  them  especially  sensitive 
to  motion,  sleep  amid  a  swaying  of  branches  that  sometimes 
becomes  quite  violent  without  disturbing  them.  Our  knowledge 
of  the  reasons  why  motion  induces  sleep  seems  to  me  still  too 
limited  to  justify  dogmatism  on  the  subject. 

In  the  case  of  my  niece,  the  nurse  and  grandmother  being  at 
one  in  advice,  the  mother  decided  to  make  no  attempt  to  teach 
her  to  go  to  sleep  alone.  During  the  first  two  months,  she  usually 
dropped  to  sleep  at  the  breast ;  at  other  times  the  nurse  easily 
lulled  her  by  patting.  No  one  else  seemed  very  successful  with 
this  method,  but  in  the  second  month  the  motion  of  the  baby- 
carriage  usually  brought  sleep.  As  I  have  said  above  (p.  119)  the 
rougher  motion  proved  the  more  soothing,  and  even  if  the  baby 
had  been  fretting  as  the  carriage  crossed  the  smooth  veranda,  her 
complaints  would  stop  as  soon  as  the  wheels  touched  the  gravel 
walk,  and  she  would  lie  placidly  till  her  lids  dropped  in  sleep.     At 


Shinn.] 


The  Development  of  a  Child.  265 


this  time  too,  (latter  part  of  the  second  month,)  when  she  now  and 
then  had  to  be  coaxed  to  sleep  without  the  carriage,  the  most 
efficient  way  proved  to  be  to  lay  her  along  my  knees,  her  feet  against 
my  body,  her  head  resting  in  my  palm,  and  to  trot  her  with  a  short, 
joggling  motion,  crooning  monotonously;  this  process  would  send 
her  to  sleep  even  when  she  was  crying  quite  hard.1  She  went  to 
sleep  in  driving  more  easily  than  when  wheeled  in  the  baby- 
carriage. 

In  the  third  month,  she  was  fond  of  being  held  up  against  her 
mother's  right  shoulder,  so  that  she  could  get  her  left  thumb  into 
her  mouth,  and  would  then  lie  contentedly  and  go  to  sleep. 
Sucking  the  thumb,  however,  was  not  very  firmly  associated  with 
sleep,  for  she  sucked  it  also  when  wide-awake.  About  the  middle 
of  this  month  she  was  given  a  small,  closed  rubber  nipple,  which 
she  soon  began  to  suck  instead ;  and  within  a  week  this  became 
firmly  associated  with  sleep.  With  this  third  month,  also,  the 
mother,  wishing  to  limit  the  sleep  association  to  herself,  took 
exclusive  charge  of  putting  the  baby  to  sleep  (rocking  in  a  chair 
and  singing  softly,  the  old-fashioned  way);  and  by  the  83d  day 
evidence  appeared  that  the  association  was  somewhat  established, 
when  the  baby,  who  was  fretting  in  her  grandmother's  arms, 
nestled  down  contentedly  and  went  to  sleep  in  her  mother's.  (It  is 
to  be  remembered  that  she  did  not  know  her  mother  by  sight  at 
this  time,  and  had  no  preference  for  her,  except  as  she  recognized 
her  touch  and  manner  of  holding  in  connection  with  sleep  and 
feeding;  indeed,  as  a  rule,  she  had  preferred  her  grandmother's 
arms.)  The  89th  day,  her  mother  was  occupied  for  the  afternoon, 
and  wished  to  delegate  the  putting  to  sleep  for  once:  but  after 
being  entirely  happy  in  my  charge  for  an  hour  or  two,  when  the 
baby  grew  sleepy  she  cried  for  her  mother,  and  would  not  let  me 
put  her  to  sleep,  nor  be  consoled  till  her  mother  took  her. 

In  the  fifth  month,  the  rocking  and  soft  singing  failed,  and  the 

1  It  is  said  by  experienced  nurses  that  babies  who  refuse  to  go  to  sleep 
when  rocked  smoothly,  ate  sometimes  readily  soothed  byjolting.  I  have  been 
told  of  one  baby  who  had  all  the  evening  resisted  efforts  to  put  her  to  sleep,  till 
in  despair  her  grandmother  took  her,  and  sitting  down  in  a  kitchen  chair  began 
to  tilt  it  back  and  forth,  from  front  legs  to  back  ;  and  this  racking  motion 
promptly  sent  the  baby  to  sleep.  Another  baby  would  not  go  to  sleep  in  a 
cradle  till  a  stick  of  wood  was  put  under  the  rockers,  so  that  the  cradle  jolted 
heavily  at  every  swing. 


266  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

regular  process  of  putting  to  sleep  was  to  seat  the  baby  upright  on 
her  mother's  knee,  give  her  the  little  rubber  nipple  to  suck,  and 
then  joggle  her  roughly  and  monotonously  up  and  down,  singing, 
"John  Brown  had  a  little  Injun,"  until  she  grew  drowsy;  then  to 
cuddle  her  up  in  arms  and  go  on  with  the  singing  very  softly. 
Once  in  this  month  I  find  a  note  that  she  would  not  sleep,  and 
fretted,  and  finally  cried  in  earnest,  until  her  mother  rose  and 
walked  with  her,  when  she  became  quiet,  and  in  less  than  a  minute 
dropped  asleep;  but  walking  with  her  was  unusual  (see  also  the 
incident  below,  p.  282). 

In  the  sixth  month  it  became  evident  that  the  use  of  the  rubber 
nipple  to  suggest  sleep  had  become  tyrannous.  It  had  been  left 
at  a  house  where  she  was  visiting,  and  she  cried  hard  at  having  to 
go  to  sleep  without  it,  and  fumbled  with  her  mouth  and  tongue  for 
it  all  over  her  mother's  face  and  shoulder.  A  week  later  she  was 
accidentally  waked  in  the  midst  of  a  nap,  and  the  nipple  was  dis- 
covered to  be  elsewhere:  she  could  not  be  got  to  sleep  again  with- 
out it  till  two  or  three  hours  later,  when,  being  very  sleepy  she  was 
at  last  coaxed  to  sleep  with  a  wooden  handle  shaped  like  the 
nipple.  The  nipple  was  not  restored  till  afternoon  of  the  next  day, 
and  every  time  that  she  went  to  sleep  in  the  interval  was  after  hard 
crying  for  five  or  ten  minutes,  or  even  half  an  hour.  The  mother 
now  determined  to  withhold  it  and  break  her  of  its  use.  The  baby 
got  through  one  night  fairly  well,  and  went  to  sleep  at  the  breast 
for  her  morning  nap,  but  lost  her  afternoon  nap  entirely;  the  next 
night  she  was  awake  crying  two  hours,  and  finally  her  mother 
surrendered  and  let  her  have  the  nipple.  The  comfort  she  expe- 
rienced in  its  use  must  have  been  quite  positive,  causing  peculiarly 
pleasant  associations,  for  twice  in  the  latter  half  of  this  month, 
when  the  nipple  was  held  up  before  her,  she  greeted  it  with  a  cry 
of  delight,  and  demonstrations  with  hands  and  feet,  —  as  I  have 
seen  babies  greet  the  sight  of  their  nursing-bottles.  This  raises 
the  question  whether  the  association  of  pleasure  with  the  nursing- 
bottle  is  so  entirely  due  to  its  connection  with  food  as  has  been 
supposed. 

With  the  growth  in  importance  of  the  nipple  as  a  sleep- 
suggester  the  trotting  ceased,  and  the  gentle  rocking  and  singing, 
with  its  help,  was  again  sufficient.     If  the  baby  waked  at  night,  to 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  26  J 

give  her  the  nipple  and  sing  a  little  was  enough,  without  taking 
her  up.  In  the  twelfth  month,  347th  day,  the  mother  tried  again 
to  break  her  of  the  use  of  the  nipple:  she  cried  hard  and  distress- 
ingly, growing  very  nervous,  and  uttering  short  sharp  wails,  as  in 
pain,  for  about  an  hour,  and  her  mother  again  surrendered;  and 
(with  the  exceptions  mentioned  below)  the  use  of  the  nipple 
remained  imperative  up  to  the  end  of  the  sixteenth  month.  At 
this  time  a  bad  cold  made  the  child  breathe  through  her  mouth,  so 
that  she  could  not  use  the  nipple;  this  cost  her  her  day  nap  for 
some  days,  but  she  was  got  to  sleep  at  night  without  it,  and 
presently  returned  to  the  day  nap  also  without  it,  and  so  dropped 
its  use  altogether  with  little  difficulty,  after  all. 

Meanwhile,  the  nipple,  singing,  and  rocking,  had  never  been 
necessary  when  driving,  though  singing  was  a  help  even  then; 
once  in  the  eleventh  month,  315th  day,  the  baby  went  to  sleep 
when  driving  after  she  had  refused  to  sleep  at  home,  even  with  the 
nipple  and  singing;  in  driving,  also,  she  would  go  to  sleep  in  any 
one's  arms,  but  at  home  not  unless  her  mother  held  her.  In  the 
ninth  month  a  nap  was  now  and  then  coaxed  by  giving  the  baby 
the  juice  of  fruit  to  suck  through  a  cloth,  but  the  device  soon  failed. 
The  266th  day  she  entirely  refused  to  be  put  to  sleep  by  her  grand- 
mother in  her  mother's  absence:  when  given  a  loquat  to  suck,  as 
soon  as  she  perceived  she  was  being  put  to  sleep,  she  cried  and 
did  not  care  for  the  loquat.  I  took  her  and  walked  with  her;  she 
sat  quietly  in  my  arms,  leaning  on  my  breast,  but  would  not  Jay 
her  head  down;  and  when  she  found  I  was  not  going  to  take  her 
outdoors,  she  began  a  low  complaining  murmur.  I  took  her  at 
last  to  the  veranda,  and  walked  up  and  down  within  the  shade. 
She  looked  very  happy  till  she  perceived  that  I  did  not  mean  to  go 
outside  that  small  beat  (on  account  of  the  extreme  heat  in  the  sun); 
then  she  began  to  look  longingly  at  her  baby-carriage  each  time 
we  passed  it  as  it  stood  on  the  veranda,  turning  her  head  to  look 
back  at  it,  till  at  last  I  put  her  into  it.  She  broke  into  a  bright 
smile,  and  was  happy  as  long  as  I  wheeled  her  about  outdoors, 
though  she  was  very  tired  and  sleepy.  She  would  not  go  to 
sleep,  however,  till  she  was  taken  driving,  and  even  then  resisted 
somewhat. 

With  the  latter  part  of  the  eleventh    month  the  need  of  her 


268  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

mother's  arms  as  a  condition  of  sleep  became  somewhat  less  imper- 
ative: on  the  324th  day  she  held  her  arms  to  come  to  me,  and  laid 
her  head  down  on  my  shoulder  as  if  to  go  to  sleep;  her  mother 
took  her  away  to  bathe  her,  but  afterward  laid  her  back  in  my  arms 
to  see  if  she  would  really  go  to  sleep  there;  she  gave  one  little  cry 
of  protest,  then  laid  her  head  down  and  went  to  sleep  in  my  arms. 
Once  this  week  when  very  sleepy  she  went  to  sleep  when  walked 
with  by  her  father.  In  the  following  months,  as  long  as  the 
daily  nap  was  kept  up,  she  repeatedly  allowed  me  to  put  her  to 
sleep  instead  of  her  mother,  but  on  the  whole  the  association  of 
sleep  with  her  mother  was  pretty  firm,  and  it  was  once  or  twice 
necessary  for  me  to  walk  with  her.  It  is  to  be  observed  that  as 
neither  the  grandmother  nor  I  could  sing,  and  had  to  substitute  a 
mere  crooning,  or  tuneless  chant,  the  absence  of  her  mother  meant 
also  the  loss  of  the  accustomed  lullaby. 

In  the  eighth  month  the  grandmother  said  that  while  driving 
with  her,  the  baby  sang  herself  to  sleep  with  a  sort  of  crooning; 
in  the  ninth  month  when  I  began  to  croon  "bylow"  to  her  while 
driving,  she  took  it  up  herself  with  a  crooning  sound,  and  at  once 
dropped  off  to  sleep.  Just  at  the  end  of  the  year  she  took  up  a 
habit  of  beginning  a  little  drawling  sound  when  sleepy  (a-a-a), 
which  she  liked  to  have  some  one  return,  then  would  drop  it  to  a 
softer  and  languider  note,  till  in  effect  she  had  sung  herself  to 
sleep.  We  found  that  when  she  began  the  notes,  if  they  could  be 
followed  up  at  once  by  comfortable  position,  quiet,  and  an  answer- 
ing crooning,  —  sometimes  a  little  swaying  to  and  fro  in  the  arms,  — 
she  would  usually  go  to  sleep.  This  did  not  become  a  fixed  habit, 
but  for  a  time  in  the  thirteenth  month  it  interrupted  the  use  of  the 
nipple,  which  of  course  she  could  not  suck  while  crooning. 

Some  weeks  before  the  use  of  the  nipple  ceased,  the  child  had 
acquired  a  new  habit  connected  with  going  to  sleep :  lying  on  her 
mother's  arm,  with  her  head  on  the  shoulder  and  one  arm  about 
the  neck,  she  would  put  up  her  hand  and  feel  for  the  hairpins  in  her 
mother's  thick  coil  of  hair,  to  pull  them  out ;  how  early  this  began 
I  did  not  notice,  but  by  the  last  week  of  the  fifteenth  month  she 
could  not  go  to  sleep,  either  in  my  arms  or  her  mother's,  unless  she 
was  pulling  out  the  pins,  and  even  after  her  lids  had  fallen  the  little 
hand  would  still  fumble  in  the  hair.     I  failed  also  to  note  the  pass- 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  269 

ing  away  of  the  habit:  it  must  have  lasted  a  month  or  two,  and 
doubtless  aided  in  the  emancipation  from  the  nipple.  After  this, 
the  mother  put  her  to  sleep  with  rocking  and  singing  only,  and  I 
sometimes  (after  the  eighteenth  month)  with  long  monotonous  nar- 
rations, rhymes,  counting,  etc.,  in  a  low  even  voice.  These  methods 
lasted  in  the  case  of  the  day  nap  until  it  was  given  up  in  the  twenty- 
seventh  month :  in  the  latter  months  of  the  period,  however,  they 
sometimes  failed,  and  nothing  but  a  drive  would  bring  the  nap; 
sometimes  even  the  drive  failed. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  eighteenth  month,  (seventy-seventh  week,) 
the  child  took  up,  quite  of  her  own  accord,  the  idea  of  going  to 
sleep  at  night  by  herself  in  her  crib.  I  cannot  find  that  before  the 
twentieth  month  she  actually  went  to  sleep  thus  more  than  once  or 
twice:  she  would  lie  quite  still  for  fifteen  minutes  or  so,  then  call, 
"  Mamma  ta'  Wu' !"  and  would  be  taken  up  and  rocked  to  sleep;  or 
else  she  would  throw  the  covers  off,  and  as  she  had  a  cold  at  the 
time,  her  mother  would  take  her  back  to  her  lap,  where  she  could 
keep  her  well  wrapped.  She  objected  much  to  this,  and  once  in  the 
eighty-second  week  I  heard  her  crying  and  begging  for  "own  bed!  " 
At  the  beginning  of  the  twentieth  month,  she  was  allowed  to  stay 
in  it,  and  now  fell  asleep  there  without  difficulty,  and  her  joy  in  it 
was  great;  she  seemed  to  have  a  real  feeling  of  attachment  for  the 
little  bed.  Once  when  her  mother  went  to  turn  her  over  and  cover 
her  after  she  was  asleep,  she  roused  slightly  and  murmured.  "In 
bed !  "  afraid  she  would  be  taken  out.  In  the  eighty-fourth  week 
her  mother  took  her  back  to  her  lap  for  a  night  or  two,  because 
she  insisted  in  poking  a  foot  out  to  feel  the  floor-matting  with  her 
toes;  and  she  cried  loud  and  long,  begging,  "In  bed!  "  Neverthe- 
less, in  the  last  week  of  the  month,  she  began  to  resist  going  to  bed, 
and  in  fact  going  to  sleep  anywhere,  and  had  to  be  rocked  and  sung 
to  sleep  again  for  a  fortnight.  After  this,  she  was  willing  and  even 
desirous  to  go  to  sleep  in  her  bed;  but  only  on  condition  that  her 
mother  would  stay  beside  her  and  sing.  Her  mother  wished  her 
to  go  to  sleep  alone,  and  I  often  heard  her  crying  for  an  hour  over 
this  before  she  went  to  sleep.  Once  (ninetieth  week)  she  cried  so 
long  and  hard  that  her  mother  took  her  up  and  rocked  her  to  sleep, 
and  after  that  consented  to  stay  beside  her.  For  about  a  week  the 
child  was  somewhat  uneasy  and  solicitous,  and  when  bed  was  pro- 


270  University  of  California.  [Vol.  r. 

posed  would  ask,  "Mamma  lie  down  on  mamma  bed?"  and  when 
assured  that  her  mother  would  lie  beside  her  and  sing,  would  go 
happily  to  bed;  in  her  crib,  she  would  nestle  about,  talk,  ask  for  a 
drink,  hold  her  mother's  hand,  and  finally  drop  to  sleep.  By  the 
first  night  of  the  twenty-second  month  she  was  reassured,  and 
would  ask  when  undressed  to  be  put  in  "our  dear  little  bed." 

In  the  last  quarter-year  her  mother  tried  gradually  to  accustom 
her  to  being  left  to  go  to  sleep  by  herself,  and  by  the  twenty-fourth 
month  I  find  her  submitting  to  it  without  actual  protest,  but  with 
devices  to  detain  her  mother  at  her  side,  such  as  offering  to  tell  her 
a  story.  Instances  of  this  occurred  throughout  the  third  year,  and 
indeed  are  not  unknown  at  seven  years  old.  The  mother  always 
sat  in  an  adjoining  room,  lighted,  and  separated  only  by  a  portiere, 
and  there  was  usually  talking  and  reading  going  on,  so  that  the 
child  was  well  aware  of  her  neighborhood. 

The  last  night  of  the  second  year  the  association  of  being  put 
to  bed  by  her  mother  and  having  her  at  least  close  by,  was  for  the 
first  time  broken,  and  with  unexpected  ease.  The  mother  was 
obliged  to  go  to  a  sister  who  was  ill :  the  matter  was  explained  to 
the  child,  and  she  was  asked  if  she  would  let  aunty  put  her  to  bed, 
that  mamma  might  go  and  take  care  of  poor  Aunt  May.  She  said, 
"  Ye'  "  easily,  kissed  her  mother  goodnight  with  entire  cheerfulness, 
finished  the  peach  she  was  eating,  let  me  wash  her  hands  and  face, 
and  asked  in  her  politest  way,  "Aunty,  may  Ruth  have  cubes?" 
(Hailmann  beads,  see  Sight,  Form),  played  happily  for  twenty  min- 
utes, then  consented  readily  to  go  to  bed,  kissed  the  family  good- 
night pleasantly,  and  let  me  undress  her  and  put  her  to  bed  with- 
out a  complaint.  It  is  likely  that  she  was  sustained  by  a  special 
pride  of  good  behavior  on  this  occasion  ;  and  also  by  the  privilege 
of  keeping  me  beside  her  till  she  went  to  sleep,  in  consideration  of 
her  mother's  absence.  She  asked  me  to  "lie  down  on  big  bed," 
and  tell  a  story;  I  told  a  long,  monotonous  one,  and  she  punctuated 
it  with  contented  "yeses"  till  she  dropped  off  to  sleep.  The  next 
night  she  expressed  some  desire  to  have  me  put  her  to  bed  again; 
but  the  night  after,  when  told  her  mother  must  go,  began  to  cry; 
as  soon  as  I  came  to  her,  however,  she  put  her  arms  around  my 
neck,  kissed  her  mother  goodnight  cheerfully,  and  went  to  sleep 
without  resistance  under  a  long  murmuring  story,  —  again  dropping, 
in  a  sleepy  "yes"  whenever  I  struck  a  familiar  image. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child,  27 1 

After  this,  throughout  the  third  year,  the  mother  was  obliged 
to  be  away  from  home  repeatedly  for  several  days  at  a  time,  and 
the  little  one's  crib  was  at  such  times  placed  in  my  room.  I  always 
stayed  beside  her  till  she  slept,  and  thus  had  the  opportunity  to 
observe  her  habits  in  going  to  sleep.  The  first  time  (last  week  of 
the  twenty-fifth  month)  she  went  off  to  bed  with  me  gayly,  laughing 
at  the  novelty  of  having  her  crib  upstairs,  got  into  an  irrepressible 
gale  on  my  bed,  capering  wildly,  shouting,  and  laughing;  after  she 
was  in  her  crib,  she  wished  a  story,  and  then  consented  to  go  to 
sleep  while  I  lay  near  by.  She  had  a  doll  in  her  arms,  (from  the 
sixteenth  month  she  had  now  and  then  insisted  on  going  to  sleep 
with  a  favorite  doll  in  her  arms,  and  does  still ;  but  it  never  became 
a  fixed  habit,  at  all  necessary  to  sleep,)  and  talked  to  it  a  few 
minutes,  nestling  about,  then  dropped  suddenly  asleep.  Later  she 
waked  and  cried  (see  below,  p.  286-7)  and  was  at  last  rocked  to  sleep. 
The  next  night  she  talked  a  while,  asked  for  a  story,  lay  silent  a 
while  trying  to  go  to  sleep,  prattled  a  little  to  herself,  then  flung 
herself  across  the  rail  of  her  crib,  and  putting  her  arms  around  my 
neck  as  I  lay  close  to  the  edge  of  my  own  bed,  lay  dozing,  her 
head  on  the  rail  of  her  crib,  pressed  against  my  face;  and  even  in 
her  sleep  she  would  cling  closer  if  I  moved,  and  twice,  rousing  a 
little,  kissed  my  face  and  hair;  it  was  some  minutes,  and  she  was 
fairly  asleep,  before  she  rolled  off  to  a  more  comfortable  position. 
The  next  night  she  wanted  a  book  to  "read"  in  the  dark  (as  she 
had  done  waking  in  half-light  that  morning),  prattled  a  little  from 
it  and  wished  me  to  do  so,  listened  to  a  few  Mother  Goose  rhymes, 
then,  told  she  must  lie  still  and  go  to  sleep,  prattled  to  herself  and 
wriggled  about  a  long  time  first.  So  during  several  more  nights 
that  she  slept  beside  me,  and  on  other  occasions  through  the  year 
when  she  was  with  me  for  a  few  nights,  she  accepted  me  easily  in 
place  of  her  mother,  helped  by  the  fact  that  I  placed  her  crib  more 
closely  against  my  bed,  and  lay  beside  her  till  she  slept,  so  that 
she  could  reach  over  and  cling  to  me,  or  during  the  night  could 
sometimes  creep  over  into  my  bed.  In  the  27th  month,  her  crib 
was  set  as  close  beside  her  mother's,  so  that  she  could  crawl  over 
and  cling  to  her  in  the  night,  as  she  had  been  doing  with  me.  The 
next  time  that  she  slept  beside  me,  she  whimpered  for  her  mother 
a  little,  but  as  long  as  I  talked  to  her  was  content ;  when  I  stopped, 


272  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

she  would  begin  to  call  for  her  mother,  so  I  had  to  talk  her  to 
sleep.  Two  months  later,  however,  she  was  perfectly  content 
to  go  to  sleep  beside  me,  rolled  over  and  put  her  hands  about  my 
neck,  and  asked  questions  about  owls,  bees,  etc.,  listening  drowsily 
to  the  chat  about  them,  which  I  made  low  and  monotonous,  till  she 
dropped  asleep.  It  was  my  theory  that  this  quiet  talk  tranquilized 
her  better  for  sleep  than  to  be  left  in  entire  silence. 

Once  in  the  27th  month,  when  her  mother  was  going  out 
in  the  evening,  the  child  went  cheerfully  to  bed  with  me,  but 
when  her  mother  came  in  with  wraps  on  to  kiss  her  goodnight, 
began  to  cry  for  her.  In  the  thirty-second  month,  a  young  girl 
came  to  the  house  to  take  partial  care  of  her  in  the  daytime  for  a 
few  weeks;  and  the  very  first  night  she  was  in  the  house,  only 
about  an  hour  after  her  arrival,  the  child,  who  had  so  far  made  no 
acquaintance  with  her,  dismissed  her  mother  as  soon  as  she  was 
put  in  bed,  saying,  "You  may  go  away;  Clara  will  stay  with  me," 
and  was  more  than  satisfied  to  have  the  stranger  beside  her  till  she 
slept.  In  the  thirty-third  month,  returning  from  a  camping  trip  of 
two  weeks,  for  several  nights  she  wished  me  instead  of  her  mother 
to  put  her  to  bed. 

There  seemed  to  be  in  my  niece's  case  not  merely  difficulty  in 
getting  to  sleep,  but  for  months  at  a  time  an  absolute  repugnance 
and  resistance  to  sleep.  This  seemed  at  first  due  to  a  desire  to 
play  instead  of  keeping  still.  It  did  not  appear  till  toward  the 
middle  of  the  fifth  month:  one  day  in  this  month  (134th  day)  her 
father  came  home  at  evening  while  her  mother  was  putting  her  to 
sleep,  and  interrupted  the  proceeding  in  order  to  play  with  her  for 
a  long  time.  The  next  evening  at  the  same  hour  her  mother  took 
her  to  put  to  sleep;  but  the  baby  kept  breaking  into  the  lullaby 
with  a  troubled  little  cry,  and  finally  an  incipient  wail.  We  could 
not  find  any  possible  reason,  and  concluded  it  was  a  coming  tooth 
that  pained  her;  her  mother  gave  her  a  finger  to  bite,  cuddled  and 
pitied  her,  and  at  last,  for  want  of  anything  else  to  do,  as  the  fretting 
cries  continued,  lifted  the  baby  against  her  face,  letting  her  bite  and 
mumble  at  cheek  and  chin.  At  this  the  baby  turned  and  smiled 
broadly  at  me,  and  we  perceived  that  she  had  simply  been  fretting 
for  a  frolic.     Her  mother  put  her  on  her  foot  and  swung  her  up 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  ~1 2> 

and  down,  and  she  was  joyously  content,  and  after  a  little  play 
grew  drowsy  and  went  readily  to  sleep.  No  doubt,  had  her  mother 
tried  to  force  her  to  go  to  sleep,  she  would  have  resisted  more  and 
more,  become  excited  and  sleepless,  and  lost  more  sleep  than  by 
the  concession:1  but  on  the  other  hand,  had  the  going  to  sleep  not 
been  interrupted  the  night  before,  she  would  have  slept  this  evening 
without  resistance.  The  next  day  occurred  the  reception  spoken 
of  above,  when  her  pleasure  and  excitement  kept  her  from  sleep; 
and  the  next  evening  she  cried  over  being  put  to  sleep  because  she 
was  disappointed  at  having  to  leave  a  plaything.  After  this  I  find 
recurring  notes  of  resistance  to  sleep,  and  desire  to  play  instead. 
In  the  latter  half  of  the  fifth  month  it  was  very  common  for  her 
to  wish  to  stay  in  the  family  room  and  play,  and  to  whimper 
when  taken  away  to  bed;  she  was  usually  wide  awake  and  full  of 
gayety  at  just  this  time.  The  225th  day  it  was  noticed  that  she 
babbled  more  freely  toward  bedtime.  This  same  night  her  mother 
brought  her  into  the  family  room  to  say  goodnight  before  putting 
her  to  sleep,  and  she  fairly  squealed  and  sprang  to  be  taken  by  her 
grandfather  (who  was  her  best  playmate),  and  was  very  unwilling 
to  go  back  to  her  mother,  as  if  suspecting  she  would  then  be  put  to 
sleep.  This  reluctance  to  go  to  her  mother  at  bedtime  had  been 
growing  evident  since  the  beginning  of  the  eighth  month  or  earlier, 
and   it  now  became  quite  marked.     The   229th  and   230th  days, 

1  It  is  likely  that  too  much  inflexibility  in  trying  to  force  a  wide-awake, 
playful  baby  to  sleep  ends  in  nervous  excitement  that  not  only  puts  sleep  still 
farther  off  for  the  time,  but  establishes  most  unpleasant  associations  with  the 
process  of  being  put  to  sleep,  which  will  affect  it  for  some  time.  It  is  doubtless 
good  counsel  that  care  should  be  taken  not  to  get  the  baby  wide-awake  and 
full  of  play  as  bedtime  approaches;  but  it  seems  to  me  also  good  counsel  (as 
given  by  the  grandmother)  that  when  the  baby  is  already  in  this  mood,  it 
is  better  to  let  her  get  a  little  of  the  play  out  of  her  before  trying  to  make 
her  sleep.  See  also  an  incident  of  the  239th  day  (p.  260,  above)  concerning 
the  effect  of  a  little  play  in  restoring  nervous  equilibrium  and  making  sleep 
possible.  So  too  in  the  second  year,  when  the  child  was  just  seventeen 
months  old,  she  showed  an  unusual  fretfulness  at  dinner  when  brought  in 
from  outdoors,  cried  to  go  out  again,  then  to  be  allowed  to  hold  her  own  cup 
and  spoon,  etc.,  in  a  manner  most  unlike  her.  After  she  was  undressed,  the 
evening  being  very  warm,  I  rolled  her  in  her  little  blanket  and  took  her 
outdoors  and  let  her  see  the  moon,  the  toads  hopping  about  the  garden,  and 
so  on;  she  became  quiet  and  happy,  and  was  soon  quite  content  to  come  in 
and  to  go  to  sleep. 


2  74  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

when  her  mother  tried  to  take  her  from  me  after  she  was  in  her 
nightgown,  she  whirled  about  and  clung  tightly  and  persistently  to 
me.  The  231st  day,  after  she  was  ready  for  bed,  she  was  put  down 
on  a  table,  where  she  rolled  and  played  about  till  her  mother  came 
to  get  her,  at  which  she  whirled  away,  clutched  at  the  table,  and 
buried  her  face  to  cling  there,  as  she  had  done  in  my  arms;  when 
taken,  however,  she  went  perfectly  pleasantly.  The  240th  day, 
although  she  had  had  no  morning  nap,  she  would  not  go  to 
sleep  when  wheeled  out  in  the  baby-carriage  in  the  afternoon, 
but  sat  bolt  upright  with  lids  drooping,  striving  to  keep  awake,  — 
I  thought  because  of  desire  to  enjoy  the  outing.  An  instance  of 
her  resistance  to  sleep  on  the  266th  day  has  been  given  above. 

At  the  end  of  the  tenth  month  came  a  change.  The  3<D2d  day, 
waked  in  a  nap,  she  clung  to  her  mother,  hiding  her  face  on  her 
shoulder,  and  showing  desire  to  sleep  again,  though  she  could  not; 
and  again  a  half-hour  later,  when  undressed  to  be  put  to  bed,  she 
clung  to  her  mother,  hiding  her  face  and  feeling  for  the  rubber 
nipple,  desiring  to  be  lulled,  instead  of  resisting.  During  the  next 
week  she  cried  hard,  as  before,  over  being  put  to  sleep,  and  on  the 
303d  day,  when  her  father  took  her  as  she  was  playing  with  me, 
though  she  reached  her  hands  willingly  to  go  to  him,  she  cried  out  as 
soon  as  he  turned  to  the  bedroom  door  and  reached  her  arms  back 
to  me,  and  as  the  door  shut  behind  her  broke  out  crying  bitterly. 
But  on  the  309th  day,  when  her  mother  put  the  nipple  in  her  mouth 
as  a  preliminary  to  carrying  her  off  for  her  afternoon  nap,  she  laid 
her  head  down  at  once  on  her  mother's  shoulder,  and  slept  readily; 
and  during  the  rest  of  the  eleventh  month,  and  still  more  in  the 
twelfth  month,  it  was  the  rule  (not  without  exceptions)  that  she 
went  readily  and  cheerfully  to  sleep,  if  tact  was  used  in  not  breaking 
too  suddenly  into  an  employment,  and  in  taking  her  when  she  was 
sleepy,  but  not  sleepy  enough  to  have  become  fretted.  The  324th 
day,  as  noted  above,  she  composed  herself  voluntarily  to  go  to 
sleep  in  my  arms;  the  325th  day  her  mother  told  me  that  she 
asked  to  go  for  her  nap,  uttering  begging  sounds  till  her  mother 
took  her  in  arms,  saying,  "  Shall  mamma  put  Ruth  bylow  ?  "  to 
which  the  child  answered  with  a  sound  of  satisfaction,  and  settled 
down  gladly  to  sleep.  In  the  twelfth  month  I  often  heard  her 
gladly  assent  to  the  suggestion;  in  the  fifty-first  week  when  asked, 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  275 

"Shall  we  get  the  'stopper '  [nipple,  which  was  set  in  a  stopper-like 
ivory  disk]  and  go  bylow  ?  "  she  would  nod,  and  put  out  her  hands 
and  take  the  nipple,  put  it  in  her  mouth,  and  nestle  down  in  her 
mother's  arms  in  the  usual  position  to  go  to  sleep. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  year  she  began  to  resist  the  second  daily 
nap,  keeping  resolutely  awake  all  the  afternoon,  though  very  sleepy, 
and  crying  if  any  one  tried  to  put  her  to  sleep;  and  this  resistance 
was  kept  up  till  the  afternoon  nap  was  given  up:  but  I  have  no 
other  note  of  resistance  to  sleep  till  late  in  the  fourteenth  month, 
when  I  find  mention  of  her  "Na,  na,  na !  "  shaking  or  nodding  her 
head  vigorously,  in  protest  against  bed;  in  the  sixty-first  week  I 
note  that  while  she  never  showed  anger,  there  was  a  certain  resent- 
ment at  being  taken  away  to  sleep,  and  she  would  refuse  to  kiss 
goodnight,  —  perhaps  with  a  crude  idea  that  by  doing  so  she  made 
it  sure  that  she  would  be  taken.  I  have  notes  up  to  the  latter  part 
of  the  twentieth  month  of  this  reluctance  to  bid  goodnight  when 
taken  to  bed  against  her  protest.  At  such  times,  however,  she  soon 
developed  a  considerable  desire  to  come  out  again  and  kiss  the  family 
all  round  after  being  taken  to  bed.  In  the  fifteenth  month  I  note  a 
disposition  to  cling  very  close  to  me  as  bedtime  approached.  On 
the  whole,  however,  she  went  cheerfully  to  bed  through  the  whole 
of  this  half  year,  though  usually  not  without  a  certain  amount  of 
protest ;  and  if  she  was  suddenly  taken  away  from  an  interesting 
occupation  (as  turning  over  a  favorite  basketful  of  buttons)  there 
was  sometimes  bitter  crying. 

At  eighteen  months  old,  as  mentioned  above  (p.  199),  she  once 
asked  for  "  bed  "  after  a  hard  play  ;  but  a  week  later  was  exceedingly 
unwilling  to  be  put  to  sleep,  and  cried  hard  over  it ;  and  when  her 
mother  proposed  to  tell  her  a  story,  she  began  to  cry  and  protest  at 
once,  knowing  that  it  was  intended  to  inveigle  her  into  sleeping. 
After  this,  throughout  the  third  quarter  of  the  year,  she  became  again 
very  averse  to  going  to  bed ;  some  vigorous  protests  against  the 
nap,  too,  or  absolute  refusal  of  it,  are  noted.  In  the  eighty-first  and 
eighty-second  weeks,  when  her  mother  came  to  take  her  to  bed, 
she  would  cling  to  me,  sometimes  stiffening  her  back  with  a  little 
temper  and  crying  out  in  intense  protest.  "Aunty  come  back!" 
she  would  call  after  me  in  the  twentieth  month  when  I  left  her  in 
her  mother's  lap;  "Aunty  take  Ruth  more!"  when  her  mother 
19 


276  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

came  to  take  her,  —  clinging  and  cuddling  to  me,  her  face  pressed 
against  mine.  In  the  twentieth  and  twenty-first  months  it  was  not 
uncommon  for  her  to  cry  bitterly,  sometimes  for  an  hour,  over  being 
put  to  sleep.  Once  in  the  ninetieth  week,  brought  indoors  to  be  put 
to  bed,  she  was  no  sooner  set  down  on  the  floor  than  she  made  a 
dash  first  for  one  door  and  then  for  the  other.  When  caught, 
however,  she  gave  up  and  went  to  bed  graciously  enough.  In 
other  cases,  after  some  loud  scolding  and  protesting,  she  would 
go  quite  cheerfully  to  bed.  One  evening  in  the  eighty-eighth  week, 
she  whimpered  to  go  to  her  mother  from  her  father  after  she  was 
undressed,  and  after  consenting  to  kiss  me  goodnight  dismissed 
me  peremptorily  with,  "  Go 'way !  "  and  as  I  lingered  outside  the 
door  to  see  her  settle  herself  for  sleep,  added  sternly,  "Shut  door  !  " 
The  recurrence  of  aversion  to  going  to  bed  in  this  quarter-year 
was  very  likely  connected  with  the  transition  from  going  to  sleep 
in  her  mother's  lap  to  going  to  sleep  in  the  crib.  When  the  ques- 
tion of  method  was  settled  to  her  liking,  the  aversion  seems  to  have 
disappeared;  at  least,  I  find  no  note  of  it  afterward. 

In    the    third   year,    I    find   scarcely   a    hint    of    resistance    to 
going  to  bed;  nothing  more   than    that   she  now  and    then  went 
"reluctantly;"   and    once  in    the   twenty-ninth    month,    when    her 
father  came  to  take  her  to  bed,  she  tried  to  put  him  off  with,  "I 
busy."     My  record  in  the  third  year,  taxed  by  the  increasing  com- 
plexity of  psychical  development,  is  fir  from  full  in  these  matters, 
and  such  an  incident  is  doubtless  but  one  of  many  :  but  I  have  notes 
enough  that  show  a  general  habit  of  cheerful,  readiness  to  go  to 
bed.     In  the  twenty-fifth  month,  e.g.,  "  Her  mother  calling  her  to 
come  to  bed,  she  rose  obediently,  kissed  us   all,  and  trotted  off 
tugging  chair  and  doll."     In  the  first  week  of  the  twenty-seventh 
month,  immediately  after  supper,  she  announced  that  she  wished  to 
"go  straight  to  bed,"  —  probably  because  she  was  to  sleep  upstairs 
with  me,  a  novelty;  it  was  an  hour  before  her  time,  but  she  stuck  to 
it,  and  went  happily  to  bed.     A  week  later  she  seemed  sleepy  at 
dinner  and  I  asked  if  she  would  like  to  go  to  bed.     "  Yes.     And 
have  my  clothes  off,  —  so,"  showing  in  a  pantomime.     She  was 
firm  in  the  purpose,  and  was  undressed  and  went.     In  the  second 
week  of  the  thirty-second  month  I  note  that  she  habitually  asks  to 
be  undressed  and  put  to  bed  about  8  o'clock.     (Her  bedtime  had 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  277 

always  been  later  than  that  of  most  babies  on  account  of  her  father's 
late  return  by  train  ) 

To  some  extent  in  the  second  year,  and  still  more  in  the  third, 
she  Avas  disposed  to  caper  and  play  when  undressed,  and  had  to  be 
kept  under  some  repression  even  after  she  was  in  her  crib.  In  the 
early  part  of  the  twenty-seventh  month  she  regarded  it  as  a  joke  to 
keep  putting  out  a  foot  or  hand  from  the  warm  crib  into  the 
December  air.  Later  the  same  month,  I  went  in  one  evening  after 
she  had  been  left  in  bed,  and  found  her  jumping  round  on  her 
mother's  bed  in  great  spirits.  "Did  mamma  say  you  might?"  I 
asked.  "No,"  said  the  child  composedly:  "she  sai',  'Tay  i'  you 
li'  be'  "  ("she  said,  Stay  in  your  little  bed  ").  Her  mother  coming 
in  at  the  moment  ordered  her  back  into  bed,  and  after  ling-erin"-  as 
long  as  she  dared,  she  obeyed  ;  then,  "  Mamma,  I  want  a  drink  !  " 
As  soon  as  her  mother  was  gone  for  water,  she  climbed  out  and 
began  capering  round  again.  "  Ruth !  "  said  her  mother  sternly, 
returning.  "  Here !"  called  the  child  with  a  dramatically  innocent 
tone.  She  scrambled  back  to  bed  at  the  last  moment  she  dared, 
and  then  proposed  to  "  give  aunty  part  of  my  drink,"  as  a  pretext 
for  another  excursion. 

Reluctance  to  go  to  bed  even  in  the  first  year,  and  still  more  in 
the  second,  could  often  be  allayed  by  certain  ceremonies:  thus  in  the 
latter  part  of  the  fourteenth  month  she  "could  be  reconciled  even  to 
going  to  bed"  by  being  set  to  waving  "by-by"  with  a  handkerchief, 
a  highly  valued  accomplishment.  At  eighteen  months  old  she 
attached  great  importance  to  my  carrying  her  into  the  bedroom 
and  laying  her  down  in  her  mother's  lap  to  be  undressed,  and 
would  give  up  her  occupations  and  go  contentedly  on  this  condi- 
tion: the  next  week  she  wished  me  to  take  her  after  she  was 
undressed  and  lay  her  in  her  mother's  lap,  —  the  more  purely  a 
ceremony,  as  she  was  then  laid  at  once  in  her  crib;  it  would  not 
answer  at  all,  however,  for  her  mother  to  lift  her  in  the  first  place 
and  put  her  in  her  crib.  Later  in  the  month,  though  she  would 
cry  out  in  protest  at  being  taken  from  me  to  put  to  bed,  she  would 
become  quite  content  if  I  would  agree  to  go  with  her  and  sit  by 
while  she  was  undressed;  next  (twentieth  month)  she  wished  me 
nightly  to  come  in  and  lay  her  in  her  crib.  Once  (twenty-first 
month)  being  arrested  on  the  stairs  while  bringing  down  a  little. 


278  University  of  California.  [vol.  i. 

bucket  with  shells  in  it,  she  resisted  strongly,  crying,  "Want  go  up- 
stairs!" and  protesting  against  the  idea  of  bed;  but  when  she  found 
protest  vain,  she  asked  that  she  might  take  the  bucket  of  shells  with 
her,  and  when  this  was  granted,  became  somewhat  reconciled.  In 
the  twenty-seventh  month,  though  carried  off  to  bed  while  her 
interest  in  the  striking  of  an  old  clock  that  she  had  been  allowed  to 
experiment  with  was  still  intense,  she  made  no  resistance,  the  sug- 
gestion, "Say,  'Goodnight,  little  clock,'"  being  enough  to  reconcile 
her  to  parting,  as  a  similar  one  usually  did  at  the  time. 

The  increase  of  disposition  to  affection  and  confidence  at  bed- 
time was  very  perceptible  to  me  in  the  third  year,  as  it  had  been 
earlier  to  her  mother.  Her  willingness  in  the  latter  months  of  the 
first  year  to  nestle  in  her  mother's  arms,  or  even  mine,  to  be  put  to 
sleep,  had  showed  the  rudiment  of  this,  since  she  was  ordinarily  an 
independent  child,  not  disposed  to  be  held  or  caressed.  It  was 
clearly  evident  in  her  desire  to  keep  her  mother  by  her,  to  hold  her 
hand  and  feel  her  companionship,  in  the  latter  half  of  the  second 
year.  Several  times  during  this  period,  when  her  mother  refused 
her  hand  on  cold  nights,  the  child  begged  to  be  allowed  instead  the 
end  of  her  mother's  long  braid  of  hair,  and  went  to  sleep  clinging 
to  that.  The  desire  of  my  attendance  to  lay  her  in  her  mother's 
lap,  etc.,  had  some  element  of  the  same  increased  sense  of  need  for 
companionship:  she  was  not  especially  loath  to  part  with  me  at 
other  times.  Several  instances  have  been  given  above  of  her  cling- 
ing behavior  when  she  slept  with  me  in  the  third  year,  and  many 
others  might  be  given.  Once,  c.  g.,  at  the  end  of  the  twenty-fifth 
month,  when  I  blew  out  the  light,  lying  on  the  bed  beside  her,  she 
presently  rolled  over,  felt  for  me  and  hugged  me  a  few  moments, 
then  back  to  her  nestling  and  prattling  to  herself,  —  not  as  if  she 
felt  fear  in  the  dark,  and  wished  to  assure  herself  of  my  protection, 
but  as  if  she  experienced  a  wave  of  affection  in  the  consciousness 
of  my  companionship  in  the  darkness.  So  too  during  the  night:  in 
this  same  week,  e.  g.,  finding  her  uncovered  and  chilled,  I  wished  to 
take  her  into  my  bed;  she  refused,  and  cried  and  resisted  when  I 
tried  to  lift  her  in,  so  I  waited  till  she  had  dropped  asleep,  then 
moved  her;  and  when  she  was  once  there,  rousing  from  time  to 
time,  she  clung  sleepily  to   me,  felt  with   her  arms  to  put  them 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  279 

around  my  neck,  patted  my  hair  and  face,  and  once  felt  with  her 
lips,  half  asleep,  to  kiss,  murmuring  my  name,  then  dropping  asleep 
again.  Even  as  early  as  the  eighteenth  month  I  find  instances 
of  willingness  to  curl  up  in  one's  arms  for  a  confidential  chat  at 
bedtime;  and  at  twenty-five  months  I  found  her  in  an  affectionate 
and  acquiescent  mood  for  a  talk  on  misdoings  of  the  day. 

There  was  much  that  was  quite  striking  in  her  behavior  on 
awaking  from  sleep.  The  first  note  that  I  have  on  the  subject 
records  a  condition  of  high  general  felicity  after  sleep:  this  was  on 
the  32d  day,  when,  having  slept  very  late,  and  being  comfortable 
otherwise,  the  baby  was  jollier  than  I  had  ever  seen  her,  smiling 
over  and  over.  After  the  long  sleep  (8  hours)  of  the  56th  night, 
she  was  very  active  about  holding  up  her  head  and  insisting  on 
being  given  a  sitting  position,  —  yet  perhaps  not  more  so  than 
usual:  after  the  long  sleep  of  the  61st  night  (9^  hours)  she  was  in 
high  good  humor  all  the  morning,  and  showed  delight  in  some  col- 
ored strips  with  which  "we  had  before  failed  to  attract  her  attention. 
Throughout  the  whole  record  I  have  notes  of  contentment  or  jollity 
after  a  good  sleep.  On  the  other  hand,  there  was  sometimes 
noticeable  fretfulness  after  a  nap:  I  note  it  but  once  in  the  first 
five  months  (47th  day) ;  but  from  the  beginning  of  the  sixth  month 
I  find  repeated  notes  of  a  fretful  mood  and  disposition  to  cry,  last- 
ing sometimes  a  few  minutes,  sometimes  for  hours,  after  a  nap. 
The  reason  for  such  difference  of  moods  was  not  clear,  but  I  have 
made  some  conjectures  concerning  it,  as  will  be  seen  below. 

That  the  baby  should  often  whimper  on  waking  was  to  be 
expected  in  the  earliest  months,  as  she  was  likely  after  the  interval 
of  sleep  to  be  uncomfortable  from  hunger,  thirst,  or  wetness.  Yet 
after  the  cj'^-hour  sleep,  an  extremely  long  interval,  she  did  not 
cry,  and  showed  hunger  only  by  vigorous  sucking  of  her  fist 
This  made  me  suspect  that  the  discomfort  sometimes  shown  on 
waking  might  include  a  little  remnant  of  sleepiness,  even  when  she 
seemed  to  have  waked  quite  spontaneously :  the  sleep  had  perhaps 
been  broken  in  its  light  final  stage  by  some  imperceptible  disturb- 
ance before  the  baby  had  quite  had  enough.  I  have  a  note  on  the 
105th  day  that  she  was  waked  in  the  middle  of  a  nap  by  being 
bundled  off  the  cars,  and  took  it  with  entire  cheerfulness;  but  it  was 
of  course  exceptional  not  to  show  discomfort  from  a  broken  nap. 


280  University  of  California.  LVol.  i. 

On  the  same  day  (33d)  that  I  noted  in  the  morning  her  unusual 
display  of  smiles,  she  wept  at  noon  her  first  tears,  having  waked 
alone  and  cried  some  time  without  being  heard.  There  was  no 
evidence  as  yet  that  this  unusual  distress  was  due  to  anything  more 
than  the  continuance  of  the  uncomfortable  sensations  that  had 
caused  the  crying;  but  it  did  become  evident  soon  that  to  wake 
and  cry  awhile  alone,  without  bringing  help,  gave  her  a  distress 
that  had  an  element  of  terror.  Being  left  alone  in  itself  she  did 
not  always  mind,  —  she  lay  contentedly  alone  in  a  room  for  a  half 
hour  while  the  family  went  to  breakfast,  on  the  37th  day;  nor  even 
waking  alone  in  itself,  —  as  when,  on  the  1 1 2th  day,  she  waked 
before  light  in  the  morning  (alone,  so  far  as  she  could  perceive,  in 
the  dark)  and  announced  herself  by  a  series  of  loud  and  joyous 
croaks.  It  was  only  after  crying  awhile  alone  and  unnoticed  that  the 
note  of  panic  appeared.  In  the  last  week  of  the  fourth  month, 
the  1 19th  day,  she  waked  alone  in  the  dark,  and  had  been  crying 
some  time  before  her  mother  heard  her  and  brought  her  out  to 
where  we  were  at  dinner,  and  put  her  into  her  high-chair,  where 
she  was  usually  very  happy  in  the  lights  and  sounds.  She  had 
evidently  grown  frightened  while  crying  alone  and  was  not  yet  fully 
reassured,  and  was  quite  unwilling  to  leave  her  mother's  arms;  she 
kept  leaning  toward  her  from  the  high-chair  (she  had  not  yet 
learned  to  hold  out  her  arms)  and  uttering  troubled  cries;  then  as  it 
was  some  minutes  before  her  mother  took  her,  she  grew  more  and 
more  disturbed,  till  she  was  crying  in  real  fright  again,  and  was 
soothed  with  difficulty.  The  next  afternoon  at  about  the  same 
time,  waking  alone,  she  began  at  once  to  cry  with  the  note  of 
fright,  and  was  hard  to  soothe.  I  did  not  think  the  dark  had  much 
to  do  with  her  fear,  except  as  it  helped  to  recall  the  impressions  of 
the  afternoon  before.  The  I22d  day,  after  there  had  been  time  for 
this  association  to  fade,  she  waked,  and  even  cried  alone  in  the  dark 
for  a  while  before  she  was  heard,  without  showing  any  of  the  fear 
of  a  few  days  before:  but  she  was  still  in  a  condition  of  happy 
excitement  from  events  of  the  day  before,  and  as  noted  above 
(p.  258)  both  on  this  occasion  and  twice  in  the  fifth  month,  though 
unable  to  sleep  much,  she  always  waked  in  joyous  spirits  while  the 
effect  of  the  excitement  remained. 

In  the  last  week  of  the  fifth  month  (147th  day)  she  waked  in 


Shinn  i  The  Development  of  a  Child.  28  I 

the  evening  and  whimpered,  and  her  mother  being  absent,  her 
father  took  her  up  and  attempted  to  soothe  her.  This  was  a  new 
proceeding,  and  evidently  excited  a  feeling  of  fear,  and  by  the  time 
her  increasing  cries  had  summoned  grandmother  and  aunt,  she  was 
in  a  good  deal  of  panic,  crying  tears,  and  was  with  some  difficulty 
quieted  and  diverted  until  her  mother's  return,  some  twenty  minutes 
later.  Once  in  the  sixth  month,  164th  day,  she  showed  fear  when 
I  took  her,  in  the  dark,  from  her  mother's  arms  just  after  she  had 
waked;  but  when  I  had  brought  a  light,  she  recognized  me  with 
a  smile. 

It  was  evident  that  so  far  there  had  been  only  occasional  alarms 
on  account  of  some  special  cause  just  after  sleep;  but  in  the  latter 
part  of  the  seventh  month  I  noticed  that  she  was  habitually  troubled 
and  timid  on  first  waking,  clinging  to  her  mother  and  ready  to  cry 
pitifully  if  taken  from  her.  This  I  note  over  and  over  in  the  first 
half  of  the  eighth  month.  In  the  tenth  month  she  usually  cried 
hard  immediately  on  waking,  though  by  the  eleventh  month  a  sort 
of  troubled  whimpering  of  "  Mam-mam-mam"  was  commoner.  It 
seemed  strange  that  so  happy  a  child  should  so  rarely  wake 
happy,  and  I  felt  satisfied  that  there  was  some  confusion  of  mind 
in  returning  to  consciousness  that  caused  fear  or  distress,  which 
was  deepened  by  finding  herself  alone,  as  she  commonly  did  on 
waking  from  a  nap,  and  that  it  would  be  well  if  some  one  could 
always  be  in  sight  when  she  waked,  —  at  least  until  associations  of 
confidence  and  familiarity  could  replace  the  unpleasant  ones  that 
had  gathered  about  the  moment  of  waking.  In  the  eleventh 
month,  325th  day,  I  found  an  opportunity  to  step  at  once  to  her 
at  the  first  stir  of  waking,  and  found  her  lifting  her  head  from  the 
pillow  and  saying,  "  Mam-mam,"  in  a  whimpering  tone.  When  she 
saw  me,  as  I  stood  quietly  beside  the  crib,  she  laid  her  head  down 
again  for  nearly  a  minute,  as  if  she  would  sleep  again;  then  began 
to  nestle  and  complain,  got  up,  holding  to  the  edge  of  the  crib,  and 
clung  to  me,  and  presently  held  out  her  arms,  and  when  I  took  her, 
she  seemed  at  once  happy  and  sunny.  In  the  afternoon  her  mother 
took  her  up  when  she  cried  on  waking  from  her  nap,  and  carried 
her  into  the  next  room ;  she  seemed  sleepy  and  shy,  but  almost  at 
once  pointed  to  a  picture  that  she  knew,  and  then  to  me,  with 
a  curious    effect   of  rediscovering    her  world,  —  coming   back  to 


282  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

recognition  of  her  surroundings,  —  and  soon  was  bright  and  happy 
again.  This  behavior  confirmed  my  impression  that  she  passed 
through  some  stage  of  intimidating  confusion  on  the  borderland 
of  sleep,  —  due  doubtless  to  slow  re-establishment  of  cerebral 
circulation,  or  to  disturbances  of  general  sensation,  but  just  what 
sort  of  confusion  would  be  hard  to  guess  in  the  case  of  such  a  little 
thing,  —  and  that  she  found  a  familiar  face  and  protecting  arms  quite 
a  rope  to  cling  to  while  she  made  her  way  back  to  firm  ground  of 
waking  consciousness. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  eleventh  month,  though  she  still  cried 
when  she  waked  alone,  she  seemed  growing  less  timid  than  she 
had  been.  But  the  3 32d  day  she  waked  alone  in  the  dark,  while 
her  father  was  talking  loudly  in  a  neighboring  room  (perhaps  was 
waked  by  the  sound  of  his  voice,  and  did  not  recognize  it  through 
the  wall)  and  cried  some  time  before  she  was  heard.  When  she 
was  taken  up,  she  quieted  a  little,  then  began  crying  again,  and 
cried  harder  and  harder,  quieting,  then  breaking  out  again,  for 
nearly  an  hour:  she  never  had  cried  so  in  her  life  except  once 
for  a  colic,  an  unfamiliar  ailment  which  seemed  to  cause  her  more 
terror  than  pain  (see  Pain,  p.  154,  footnote).  The  efforts  of  the 
concerned  and  sympathetic  family  to  divert  her  probably  added  to 
her  excitement;  she  clung  to  her  mother,  would  have  no  one  else 
and  cried  least  when  her  mother  walked  about  with  her  in  a 
brightly  lighted  room,  talking  softly  and  cheerfully  to  her.  At 
last,  when  she  was  quite  tired  out,  her  grandmother  brought  her 
a  drink  of  water;  she  took  it  gratefully,  and  almost  at  once  fell 
asleep.  During  the  night  she  waked  often,  crying.  For  some 
days  after  this  her  crying  on  waking  alone  was  invariable;  but 
on  the  338th  day  she  slept  three  hours  later  than  usual  in  the 
morning,  and  on  waking  made  no  cry,  but  tried  to  help  herself  to 
the  cup  of  milk  left  standing  beside  her  crib  since  the  midnight 
meal,  and  only  after  spilling  the  remnant  of  this  did  she  raise  her 
voice.  Her  father  came  to  her  then,  and  she  instantly  reached 
her  arms  to  him  and  pointed,  begging  to  be  taken  outdoors.  She 
was  afterward  unusually  happy  in  the  bath.  This  incident  renewed 
my  impression  of  the  second  month  that  if  sleep  could  be  absolutely 
finished  out  to  the  end,  the  state  of  confusion  and  uneasiness  would 
not  intervene  between  sleep  and  waking.     The  moral  of  this  is  that 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  283 

it  is  well  to  guard  the  quiet  of  a  baby's  sleep  and  prolong  it  to  the 
utmost,  instead  of  trying  to  accustom  him  to  sleep  through  noises. 
In  the  later  years  of  childhood  (see  pp.  255-6,  above)  sleep  is  so 
much  sounder  than  in  infancy  that  if  such  training  is  necessary 
(and  of  course  sleepers  cannot  expect  to  go  through  life  in  a 
cloistered  seclusion  from  sounds)  it  can  be  begun  then. 

In  the  early  months  of  the  second  year  the  habit  of  waking  in 
distress  rather  increased  than  lessened.  In  the  thirteenth  month  I 
note  that  the  child  always  wakes  crying  most  bitterly,  as  if  in 
grief  or  pain.  In  the  last  week  of  the  month  (fifty-sixth  week)  she 
waked  from  a  nap  prematurely  and  was  very  willing  to  let  me 
put  her  to  sleep  again:  she  did  not  go  soundly  enough  to  sleep t 
however,  to  be  laid  down,  so  I  sat  holding  her  till  she  waked.  She 
opened  her  eyes  and  looked  into  my  face  with  a  startled  expression 
at  first ;  then  she  sat  up  and  pointed  to  a  picture  of  a  dog  on  the 
wall,  saying,  "Wow-wow!"  (again  as  if  relocating  herself  by  means 
of  a  familiar  object.)  She  was  willing  at  once  to  be  put  on  the 
floor,  went  directly  to  her  toys,  and  was  much  interested  in  them; 
there  was  no  interval  of  crying  and  clinging,  and  I  was  disposed 
to  attribute  it  to  the  fact  that  she  had  opened  her  eyes  to  a 
familiar  face. 

I  had  not  hitherto  attributed  her  distress  in  waking  to  any  fear 
of  the  dark,  though  darkness  doubtless  heightened  timidity  from 
other  causes.  I  now  noticed,  with  the  beginning  of  the  fourteenth 
month,  that  when  she  waked  and  cried  in  the  dark,  she  was  not 
easily  soothed  unless  a  light  was  taken  when  one  went  to  her.  I 
shall  discuss  this  point  of  fear  of  the  dark  more  fully  under  the 
head  of  Fear;  but  as  far  as  it  was  especially  connected  with  waking, 
it  seemed  to  be  mainly  an  acquired  fear,  due  to  her  having  always 
gone  to  sleep  with  a  light  in  the  room,  so  that  when  she  waked  the 
conditions  were  strangely  changed  from  those  of  her  last  waking 
consciousness,  the  people  and  things  she  knew  had  disappeared, 
and  she  had  had  no  experience  to  lead  her  to  think  of  them  as  still 
present,  as  she  would  have  done  had  she  been  accustomed  to  the 
dark  while  awake.  She  waked,  so  to  speak,  to  find  herself  lost  in 
the  darkness.  It  seems  to  me  desirable  on  this  account,  if  no 
other,  that  babies  should  be  put  to  sleep  in  a  dark  room. 

During-  the  fifteenth  and  sixteenth  months  I  find  no  note  about 


284  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

her  behavior  on  waking,  and  in  the  seventeenth  one  or  two  refer- 
ences to  her  waking  very  sunny  in  the  morning,  spilling  over  with 
jollity  and  activity,  when  the  night's  sleep  had  been  long  and  full. 
She  would  be  notably  clear  in  mind  after  such  a  waking,  recalling 
her  small  vocabulary  with  facility,  and  talking  more  than  usual. 
After  a  daytime  nap,  on  the  other  hand,  either  distressed  crying  or 
a  condition  of  timidity  and  mental  arrest  seems  to  have  been  com- 
mon (though  my  notes  are  not  full  or  explicit  on  the  point).  One 
afternoon,  at  the  beginning  of  the  nineteenth  month,  she  had  just 
waked  from  a  nap,  and  was  sitting  on  her  mother's  lap  when  I  came 
in  from  the  city.  She  greeted  me  with  a  habitual  and  almost 
mechanical  question  of  "Papa?"  (he  sometimes  returned  on  the 
same  train  with  me  and  sometimes  not,  so  that  it  was  a  regular 
subject  of  inquiry),  but  paid  no  farther  attention  to  my  entrance. 
Presently  her  grandmother  kissed  her:  on  which  she  put  up  her 
lip  and  almost  cried,  —  not  peevishly,  nor  in  annoyance,  but  in  a 
pathetic  way,  as  if  her  feelings  were  hurt.  I  tried  it,  and  then  her 
mother,  with  the  same  result.  She  would  not  look  at  us  nor  heed 
questions,  but  sat  looking  absently  before  her,  eating  a  cracker  (she 
had  waked  hungry)  and  seeming  as  if  her  brain  were  dormant.  At 
last  something  we  said  to  each  other  interested  her,  and  she  slowly 
came  out  of  her  absent  condition  and  became  like  herself. 

At  the  beginning  of  the  twenty-first  month,  her  fretfulness  and 
crying  on  waking  from  a  nap  were  surprising:  the  611th  day,  for 
instance,  she  lay  in  my  arms  for  many  minutes,  breaking  into  crying 
at  the  least  crossing  of  a  whim,  —  c.  g.,  having  been  given  a  cracker, 
she  asked  for  "  one  in  the  other  hand;"  her  mother  broke  the  one 
she  had  in  two,  and  put  a  piece  in  each  hand,  as  an  easy  way  of 
gratifying  the  fancy,  (which  was  common  at  the  time  and  did  not 
seem  to  be  any  wish  for  a  double  amount  of  cracker,  but  merely 
regard  for  symmetry);  but  the  child  broke  into  violent  crying,  and 
could  not  be  consoled  until  another  cracker  was  brought.  Such 
crying,  I  noted  at  the  time,  was  never  heard  from  her  except  just 
after  sleep  and  when  her  mother  was  trying  to  put  her  to  sleep. 
Later  in  the  month,  the  621st  day,  I  stood  beside  her  when  she 
waked  from  a  nap,  and  had  my  arm  about  her  before  she  was  fairly 
awake.  She  did  not  cry,  nor  show  the  frequent  distress  and 
fretfulness;    but   seemed  to  go  through  a  stage  of  confusion  and 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  285 

discomfort  in  waking,  staring  in  a  startled  way;  rose  to  her  knees 
with  her  eyes  shut,  putting  her  hands  to  her  face  and  moving 
blindly  and  bewilderedly.  Yet  she  had  no  hesitation  in  recognizing 
me,  and  came  to  me  cheerfully,  asked  for  her  mother,  but  not 
urgently,  and  when  her  mother  came,  wished  to  go  to  her,  and 
clung  about  her  neck  a  little,  but  without  fretfulness. 

In  spite  of  the  peculiar  clinging  dependence  that  appeared  when 
she  waked  crying,  I  had  not  hitherto  seen  that  the  sense  of  need 
had  attached  itself  exclusively  to  the  mother.  In  the  thirteenth 
month  (379th  day),  waking  in  the  evening  in  her  mother's  absence, 
the  child  had  allowed  me  to  take  her  without  any  timidity,  clinging 
to  me  amid  her  crying,  and  kissing  me  over  and  over,  —  though 
after  she  was  fairly  awake  and  occupied  with  something,  she  stopped 
to  say  plaintively,  "  Mamma!  "  From  that  time  up  to  the  twenty- 
first  month,  the  general  effect  of  my  notes  had  been  only  that  she 
was  apt  to  want  her  mother  at  such  times,  and  to  be  very  shy  of 
strangers.  But  on  the  626th  day,  waking  from  a  nap  in  her  mother's 
absence,  she  cried  in  good  earnest  for  her,  and  when  taken  upstairs 
and  offered  her  favorite  diversions,  she  turned  from  them  in  a  few 
seconds  to  urge  "See  mamma!"  and  broke  into  a  wail  when  told 
mamma  was  coming  back,  which  she  well  understood  as  implying 
that  for  the  present  she  was  gone.  On  this  occasion  a  loquat  finally 
consoled  her;  but  for  a  fortnight  more  I  find  notes  that  on  waking 
from  naps  she  will  have  nothing  but  "  mamma !  "  and  seems  to  find 
the  world  desolate  if  she  cannot  have  her  instantly.  Yet  her 
mother's  presence  did  not  allay  the  trouble:  even  in  her  arms  the 
child  would  cry  in  a  distressed  way. 

Meanwhile  a  habit  of  crying  wildly  in  the  night  had  begun,  —  I 
can  hardly  say  how  far  back,  as  I  was  not  with  her  at  night,  but 
certainly  by  the  twentieth  month.  One  night  in  the  latter  part  of 
that  month,  I  slept  with  her  mother,  and  took  careful  notice  of  one 
of  these  fits  of  crying.  It  was  in  a  strange  place,  but  I  could  not 
say  whether  that  had  anything  to  do  with  it,  for  she  often  cried  in 
the  same  manner  at  home.  She  asked  almost  at  once  for  the  light, 
and  her  mother  said  that  whenever  she  waked  in  the  night,  if  the 
light  was  not  promptly  lit  she  would  get  to  crying  thus.  After  she 
was  somewhat  soothed,  she  lay  for  nearly  an  hour  and  a  half  dozing 
lightly,  ready  to  wake  and  cry  if  her  mother  removed  the  arm  about 


286  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

her.  In  the  morning  her  first  words  were,  "  Light !  aunty  make 
light !  "  pointing  up  to  a  corner  of  the  room.  I  concluded  that  she 
had  seen  the  light  striking  there  from  the  candle,  which  I  had  set 
on  the  floor,  and  wondered  if  it  had  seemed  to  her  uncanny.  It 
was  evident  that  she  had  not  been  too  confused  in  mind  to  under- 
stand what  I  was  doing.  If  fear  of  solitude  (which  the  darkness 
seemed  to  create  about  her)  had  anything  to  do  with  her  behavioi^ 
it  vanished  by  day,  for  when  we  went  out,  she  scampered  fearlessly 
about  the  strange  premises,  preferring  to  run  off  alone  to  long  dis- 
tances, without  the  slightest  timidity. 

In  the  twenty-first  month  I  again  had  an  opportunity  to  observe 
the  night-crying.  She  was  in  camp  in  the  redwood  forest,  and  I 
slept  two  nights  in  the  tent  with  her  mother:  the  first  night  she 
waked  and  cried  for  almost  two  hours,  clinging  to  her  mother  and 
wailing,  "Mamma,  mamma!  "  crying  outright,  quieting,  and  break- 
ing out  again ;  her  mother  said  it  was  her  usual  behavior  in  these 
crying  fits.  The  next  night  she  waked  when  I  was  alone  with  her, 
her  mother  having  gone  with  a  lantern  to  pilot  her  father  across 
the  stream  from  the  midnight  train.  I  expected  trouble,  but  she 
allowed  herself  to  be  quieted  with  unusual  ease :  at  first  she  was 
not  altogether  satisfied  with  my  assurance  that  mamma  had  gone 
to  find  papa  at  the  choo-choo  cars,  but  the  engine  fortunately 
whistled  at  the  moment,  and  she  accepted  this  as  evidence,  observed 
contentedly,  "  Bi'  b'ow'  bea' ! "  (Big  brown  bear,  an  interesting 
denizen  of  the  little  railroad  station,  whom  her  father  took  her  often 
to  see)  and  after  a  little  more  nestling  about,  a  whimper  or  two,  and 
a  few  more  murmurs  of  "Mamma!"  dropped  asleep  in  my  arms, 
her  head  pillowed  on  my  neck.  Waking  twice  later,  she  allowed 
herself  to  be  quieted  easily  by  her  mother. 

During  the  last  quarter  of  the  year,  after  the  camping  trip,  there 
seems  to  have  been  a  good  deal  of  improvement  in  the  matter  of 
crying  at  night,  and  fretfulness  after  a  nap.  The  last  night  of  the 
year,  as  recorded  above,  her  mother  ventured  to  leave  her  all  night, 
and  when  she  awoke  in  the  night  she  accepted  her  father  instead 
with  no  great  discontent,  though  she  cried  a  little  for  her  mother. 

The  crying  fits  did  not  cease  entirely,  however,  even  in  the  third 
year.  The  first  time  that  she  was  left  entirely  to  me  at  night,  after 
going  readily  to  bed  in  my  charge,  as  related  above,  she  waked  in 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  287 

a  couple  of  hours,  crying  wildly,  —  not  for  her  mother,  nor  with 
any  discernible  cause.  She  would  make  no  answer  to  questions, 
would  cry  and  fall  asleep,  wake  in  five  minutes  and  cry  again, 
shedding  copious  tears.  There  were  perhaps  four  such  spells,  in 
one  of  which  she  groped  about  and  sobbed  for  "Boo-be'  "  (Blue- 
bell, the  doll),  but  beyond  this  she  would  say  nothing  but,  "No!" 
when  asked  any  question  or  offered  any  comfort,  —  a  drink  of 
water  or  even  a  piece  of  candy,  which  she  pushed  from  her  mouth. 
At  last  her  grandmother,  who  had  come  in,  asked  if  she  would 
come  and  be  rocked  (a  way  of  going  to  sleep  that  she  had  gladly 
given  up  months  before),  and  she  put  out  her  arms  and  went  readily, 
and  soon  dropped  asleep,  but  sobbed  in  her  sleep  for  twenty  min- 
utes or  more.  She  seemed  half  asleep  all  the  time,  and  only 
opened  her  eyes  once.  On  one  other  night,  during  a  week  that 
she  slept  beside  me  at  the  end  of  the  twenty-fifth  month,  she 
waked  crying  this  way,  but  was  easily  soothed.  On  both  these 
occasions  she  had  had  no  nap  during  the  day.  On  the  other 
nights,  though  she  waked  from  time  to  time,  she  showed  no  fear  or 
trouble. 

In  the  middle  of  the  twenty-sixth  month,  she  slept  beside  me 
again  for  four  nights.  The  first  night,  she  cried  once  on  waking, 
but  quieted  as  soon  as  I  took  her  into  bed  with  me;  the  second,  she 
roused  repeatedly,  not  waking,  but  with  a  whimpering  cry  in  her 
sleep.  In  the  morning,  she  waked  with  crying,  and  I  noticed  that 
the  first  cry  was  before  she  waked,  so  far  as  I  could  determine ;  so 
that  it  was  impossible  to  attribute  her  distress  to  fear,  or  sense  of 
loneliness,  in  finding  herself  awake  before  she  realized  that  she  was 
with  some  one.1  She  cried  several  minutes,  till  I  had  diverted  her 
with  cheerful  talk,  about  the  little  birds.  The  next  night  she  slept 
about  eleven  hours  and  a  half  without  really  waking;  she  roused 

'Such  wild  and  unaccountable  crying  at  night  is  attributed  by  medical 
writers  to  "night-terror,"  akin  to  nightmare,  due  to  sensations  of  suffocation 
from  impeded  circulation;  and  such  a  cause  —  or  perhaps  more  general  dis- 
comfort of  sensation  —  would  account  well  enough  for  the  kind  of  feeling  now 
shown;  but  it  did  not  appear  to  be  in  any  proper  sense  terror,  —  rather  a 
completely  diffused  distress,  which  she  herself  was  unable  to  refer  to  any 
grievance.  Fear  would  have  been  more  readily  relieved  by  companionship, 
for  instance.  It  is  not  easy  in  describing  it  to  define  differences,  but  it  seemed 
quite  clear  to  me  that  there  was  now,  at  least,  however  it  had  been  earlier,  no 
discernible  element  of  fear. 


288  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

and  murmured  "Aunt)'!  "  two  or  three  times,  or  uttered  a  slight 
whimper,  but  when  I  put  my  hand  on  her,  or  spoke  encouragingly, 
she  dropped  off  at  once.  Waking  in  the  morning,  she  sat  up  and 
looked  about  her  silently,  then  cried  out  that  she  heard  a  little  bird. 
She  was  perfectly  sunny  and  happy.  The  next  four  mornings,  her 
mother  told  me,  her  first  word  on  waking  was  that  she  heard  a 
little  bird,  or  that  the  little  birds  had  waked  up  and  gone  to  find 
some  seeds  for  their  breakfast. 

At  the  end  of  the  thirtieth  month  she  slept  beside  me  for  five 
nights.  The  first  night  she  slept  through  without  waking,  and  in 
the  morning  opened  her  eyes,  looked  at  me,  lay  silent  for  some 
minutes,  apparently  perfectly  content,  then  hearing  a  quail  call,  ■ — 
"Aunty,  the  little  birdies  are  waked  up!"  She  declined  to  come 
into  my  bed,  and  after  talking  a  little  of  birds,  lay  quiet  and  happy 
in  her  own  bed  till  time  for  her  bath.  In  the  succeeding  nights 
she  several  times  cried  on  waking,  without  perceptible  cause;  once 
with  heavy  sobbing  aloud;  but  she  was  always  easily  quieted. 

I  find  no  record  of  exceptional  timidity  and  trouble  at  night 
after  this,  and  though  even  in  her  seventh  year  there  have  been 
cases  of  night  crying,  it  has  been  quite  clearly  referable  to  dreams 
or  unpleasant  thoughts.  The  liability  at  night  to  the  recur- 
rence of  unpleasant  or  terrifying  imagery  from  anything  heard 
during  the  day  has  been  quite  noticeable.  She  is  not  at  all  a  timid 
child,  and  likes  stories  to  be  very  sensational  by  day,  but  some 
times  has  unpleasant  recurrences  of  them  at  night:  "I  am  not 
afraid,  but  it  is  very  disagreeable"  she  says. 

Two  curious  instances  are  recorded  of  bewildered  crying,  simi- 
lar to  that  which  was  common  on  waking,  but  in  the  period  just 
preceding  sleep.  In  the  last  week  of  the  twenty-seventh  month,  as 
I  have  mentioned  above,  she  had  gone  very  cheerfully  to  bed  in  my 
care  when  her  mother  was  going  out  for  the  evening,  but  on  her 
mother's  coming  in  to  kiss  her  goodnight  in  bed,  began  to  cry  for 
her,  and  cried  hard.  After  being  quieted  once  or  twice  and  break- 
ing out  again,  she  began  to  beg  wildly  that  I  would  dress  her  and 
take  her  downstairs;  she  was  very  sleepy  and  bewildered,  and  in 
trying  to  get  out  of  bed  scrambled  blindly  against  the  head  of  the 
bed,  and  tried  to  climb  it;  she  had  ceased  to  cry  for  her  mother 
and  cried  only  to  be  dressed.     At  last  I  took   her  from  the  bed, 


Shinn]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  289 

rolled  her  in  a  quilt,  and  sat  down  by  the  window  with  her, 
showing  her  the  moon,  and  she  quieted  and  dropped  to  sleep  in  my 
arms.  Twice  during  the  evening  she  waked  again  crying  for  her 
mother,  but  was  easily  quieted.  In  the  latter  part  of  the  twenty- 
eighth  month  occurred  a  still  more  curious  incident;  her  mother 
came  home  on  a  late  train,  after  her  bedtime,  but  she  was  allowed 
to  stay  out  of  bed  till  she  came ;  when  her  mother  came  in  and 
took  her  in  her  arms,  the  child,  who  had  asked  for  her  several  times, 
began  to  whimper  and  cling  to  her,  saying,  "I  want  my  mamma!" 
and  when  put  to  bed,  got  to  crying  hard,  saying  she  wanted  her 
mamma,  even  when  her  mother  held  her  in  arms,  and  cried  most  of 
the  time  for  two  hours,  then  dropped  asleep,  but  waked  every  half 
hour  or  so  and  cried.  Such  an  incident  seems  to  suggest  that  in 
apparent  crying  for  her  mother  at  night,  the  grievance  may  be  a 
sort  of  symbol,  a  vague  expression  of  feelings  of  loneliness  and 
timidity  which  she  does  not  understand.  Her  condition  of  health 
was  not  quite  perfect,  which  would  increase  her  normal  suscepti- 
bility to  such  emotions  at  the  time  of  sleep;  or  it  is  possible  she 
was  affected  by  the  knowledge  that  her  mother  was  going  away  on 
the  morrow  for  a  few  days,  though  she  had  not  seemed  to  mind  it 
at  the  time  she  was  told. 

In  the  third  as  in  the  second  year,  the  feeling  of  peculiar 
dependence  on  her  mother  seemed  connected  with  the  time  of 
waking,  rather  than  of  going  to  sleep.  The  first  two  mornings  that 
she  slept  beside  me,  she  cried  for  her  on  waking,  though  she  had 
not  missed  her  on  going  to  bed,  nor  in  the  night;  the  third  morn- 
ing she  asked  for  her,  but  quite  resignedly.  The  other  mornings 
of  this  sojourn  she  waked  cheerfully,  without  talking  of  her  mother: 
once  her  first  thought  was  to  greet  her  doll  joyously;  another 
morning,  as  it  grew  dimly  light,  instead  of  calling  my  name,  she 
crept  over  close  to  my  bed  and  put  her  hand  on  my  cheek,  and 
when  I  waked  and  spoke,  clung  about  my  neck,  patting  my  hair,  — 
showing  by  this  unusual  demonstrativeness  the  feeling  of  special 
need  of  companionship  in  the  waking  hour.  Once  in  this  week 
she  waked  crying  from  a  short  nap,  and  coming  to  the  foot  of  the 
stairs,  begged  and  called,  "Aunty,  come  down  ! "  In  most  instances 
she  accepted  me  thus  readily  as  a  substitute  for  her  mother  even  on 
waking,  but  not  as  easily,  on  the  whole,  as  in  going  to  bed.     During 


290  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

her  second  stay  with  me  she  waked  once  toward  morning  and  cried 
for  her  mother  a  long  time.  She  would  chat  happily  with  me 
about  the  little  birds,  etc.,  but  begin  to  cry  when  she  thought  of 
her  mother.  There  was  perhaps  a  little  upset  of  digestion  that 
increased  her  sensibility.  An  incident  later  in  this  month  showed 
how  deep  an  association  of  desolation  she  had  with  the  idea  of 
waking  up  and  crying  in  vain  for  mamma:  a  book  had  been  given 
her  in  which  was  a  little  picture  story  of  a  baby  who  waked  up  and 
cried  in  her  mother's  absence  and  was  cleverly  tended  by  a  little 
dog.  Instead  of  being  pleased,  she  found  it  deeply  pathetic,  put  up 
her  lip  and  nearly  cried  over  it,  —  a  striking  sensibility,  as  she  was 
hardly  at  all  susceptible  to  imaginative  sympathy.  Hearing  me  tell 
her  mother  of  it,  she  almost  cried  again,  repeating,  "Mamma,  baby 
did  wake  up  and  cry,  and  mamma  didn't  come!" 

During  an  absence  of  her  mother  at  the  end  of  the  twenty- 
eighth  month,  she  cried  bitterly  for  her  the  first  night,  but  was 
soon  talked  quiet  and  asleep;  the  next  night  she  fretted  very  little 
for  her.  The  first  morning,  on  waking,  she  looked  at  me  till  she 
was  satisfied  it  was  I,  then  sat  up  and  looked  anxiously  beyond  me 
in  the  bed,  and  seeing  no  one  there,  began  to  cry  for  her  mother. 
I  lifted  her  into  bed  with  me,  and  she  climbed  over  me  and  exam- 
ined, to  make  sure  there  was  no  mother  there,  and  then  broke  out 
afresh;  the  next  morning  she  cried  a  little  for  her  mother  on 
waking,  but  was  easily  diverted.  The  five  nights  that  she  was  with 
me  at  the  end  of  the  thirtieth  month,  there  was  no  crying  at  all  for 
her  mother.  The  next  time,  in  the  latter  part  of  the  thirty-second 
month,  she  once  murmured,  "Mamma!"  longingly  in  the  night. 
After  this  she  became  quite  accustomed  to  accepting  me  at  night, 
from  time  to  time. 

It  was  only  in  connection  with  sleep,  especially  with  the  waking 
from  sleep,  that  an  insistent  sense  of  need  for  the  mother  seemed 
to  be  felt  during  her  absences.  During  the  day,  she  was  occasionally 
asked  for  rather  wistfully,  but  not  greatly  missed  on  the  whole. 

From  the  first  time  the  child  slept  beside  me,  I  had  accustomed 
her  to  as  much  darkness  as  possible,  shutting  out  even  half-light 
while  she  went  to  sleep,  though  staying  close  beside  her.  She  had 
been  used  to  going  to  sleep  with  a  light  in  the  room,  and  as  early 
as  the  eleventh  month  had  refused  to  go  to  sleep  in  the  dark.     She 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  29 1 

now  soon  became  reconciled  to  it,  however,  and  the  first  night  of 
her  second  sojourn  with  me  she  objected  to  my  lighting  the  candle 
if  she  waked  in  the  night,  and  wanted  it  put  out:  "Make  dark, 
aunty,  make  dark!"  The  next  night  she  asked  to  have  it  made 
perfectly  dark  when  I  put  her  to  bed.  In  the  first  week  of  the 
twenty-seventh  month  she  slept  beside  me  for  two  nights,  and 
insisted  on  my  blowing  out  the  candle  as  soon  as  possible,  declar- 
ing, "I  like  dark!"  Both  nights  she  seemed  anxious  all  night  to 
get  as  close  to  me  as  possible,  creeping  into  my  arms,  pressing  her 
face  to  mine,  tangling  her  fingers  in  my  hair.  The  second  night 
she  came  creeping  into  my  bed  in  the  night;  I  laid  her  back  in  her 
crib  as  soon  as  she  was  asleep,  but  she  would  from  time  to  time 
rouse  and  begin  to  cry,  and  I  would  find  her  pressing  toward  me 
till  she  bumped  her  nose  against  the  side  of  her  crib,  and  when  I 
would  lean  over  to  her,  she  would  cling  about  my  neck  till  asleep, 
stirring  even  half  asleep  and  clinging  tighter  if  I  moved.  In  the 
thirty-second  month,  she  objected  to  being  left  alone  in  the  dark 
while  I  got  her  a  drink,  and  wanted  the  candle  lit  during  my  brief 
absence  from  the  room. 

I  have  been  carried  far  afield  by  this  subject;  and  the  fact  that 
I  have  been,  makes  in  favor  of  the  belief  I  have  mentioned,  that  the 
roots  of  the  emotional  relation  between  mother  and  child  are  struck 
mainly  in  the  association  of  her  with  the  vaguely  but  intensely  sus- 
ceptible states  that  border  on  sleep.  I  have  been  unable  to  exclude 
a  good  many  notes  that  concern  the  emotions,  especially  fear  and 
affection.  No  such  wide  relations  belong  to  the  subject  of  Hunger, 
for  instance:  the  mother  as  the  source  of  food  does  not  record  her- 
self in  the  baby's  nervous  system  in  any  such  manner,  to  my  obser- 
vation, as  the  mother  as  companion  and  refuge  in  the  valley  of  the 
shadow  of  sleep.  The  very  marked  increase  in  my  niece's  affection 
for  her  mother  and  longing  for  her  at  night,  visible  about  the 
twenty-seventh  and  twenty-eighth  months,  was  coincident  with  the 
placing  of  her  crib  close  beside  the  mother's  bed,  and  the  free 
climbing  over  and  clinging  to  her.  The  first  keen  imaginative 
sympathy  with  an  emotion  took  its  root  in  this  region  of  experience. 
And  the  more  closely  I  watched  and  tried  to  interpret  the  child's 
conditions  of  sensibility  at  night,  the  more  striking  seemed  to  me 
the  analogy  between  them  and  some  of  our  own  most  deep-seated 
20 


292  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

and    poetic,  but  vague,  emotions,  —  an  analogy,  indeed,  more  or 
less  recognized  always  in  literature.' 

A  few  more  definite  indications  were  noted  as  to  the  extent  or 
kind  of  confusion  in  sense  and  memory  that  accompanied  the 
waking  moments.  Once  in  the  nineteenth  month,  the  child  in 
waking  rolled  off  a  narrow  lounge,  head  first,  and  stood  on  her 
head  a  second :  I  had  sat  by  her,  somewhat  expecting  it,  and  had 
her  in  arms  at  once,  and  I  do  not  think  she  took  her  brief  inversion 
into  consciousness  at  all.  I  have  already  told  under  the  head  of 
Color  the  incident  of  her  returning  asleep  after  an  absence  of  six  and 
a  half  days,  in  the  twenty-fifth  month,  waking,  and  murmuring 
half  asleep  as  her  eyes  rested  on  me  that  I  had  on  my  brown  dress ; 
then,  looking  at  her  mother,  "Mamma,  you  need  other  dress"  (/.  e., 
want  it,  choose  it,  have  it  on,  —  you  have  changed  your  dress). 
When  she  slept  with  me  I  did  not,  even  from  the  first  time,  notice 
any  confusion  or  difficulty  in  locating  herself  on  waking:  after  the 
first  two  nights,  she  would  murmur,  "Aunty!"  at  once  when  she 
woke,  or  even  when  half  asleep;  and  going  back  to  her  mother,  she 
went  back  without  a  single  slip  to  "Mamma!"  the  instant  of  waking. 

In  the  twenty-sixth  month,  her  father  and  mother,  returning 
from  a  two  days'  excursion,  changed  cars  while  she  was  asleep,  and 
I,  chancing  to  take  the  train,  saw  her  wake  up  in  these  changed 
surroundings.  She  sat  up,  staring  about  her  wildly,  and  into  my 
face  as  I  sat  opposite,  without  a  sign  of  interest  or  recognition, 
much  as  if  I  had  been  a  bedpost;  threw  herself  back  on  her 
mother's  neck,  sat  up  again,  and  so  several  times,  gradually  waking; 
finally, pointing  before  her,  "  Mamma,  this  is  n't  our  car !  "  For  about 
a  half  hour  she  paid  no  attention  to  me,  had  little  to  say,  and 
seemed  sleepy;  then  rather  suddenly  wished  to  come  to  me,  and 
flung  her  arms  around  my  neck  with  much  affection,  —  possibly 
remembering  suddenly  that  she  had  not  seen  me  for  two  days.  At 
what  point  she  recognized  me,  or  whether  she  did  so  from  the  first, 
she  gave  no  indication.      I  often  saw  her  wake  after  a  nap  and  stare 

'The  familiar  quotation  from  In  Memoriam  occurs  at  once  to  every  reader: 

But  what  am  I  ? 
An  infant  crying  in  the  night,  — 
An  infant  crying  for  the  light, — 
And  with  no  language  but  a  cry. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  293 

wildly  around.  Other  instances  of  gradual  or  confused  waking 
occur  above. 

In  the  twenty-eighth  month  occurred  something  that  seemed  to 
show  that  sleep  obliterated  memory  of  the  immediately  preceding 
time  to  a  considerable  extent :  the  child  had  spent  the  evening  at  a 
Christmas  tree,  but  the  next  morning  she  could  recall  nothing  of  the 
evening,  covering  more  than  two  hours  before  she  went  to  sleep. 
Some  fragments  of  it  could  be  recovered  by  questioning,  but  most 
of  it  was  gone.  The  whole  two  hours  had  fallen  after  her  usual 
bedtime,  and  during  a  good  part  of  it  she  had  been  tired  and 
sleepy,  but  she  had  shown  some  enjoyment  and  interest.  Some 
months  later,  she  went  to  sleep  on  the  way  home  from  the  Park, 
but  when  some  one  asked  her  immediately  on  waking  what  she 
had  seen  at  the  Park,  she  answered  promptly,  "  You  must  n't  ride 
on  the  baby  donkey,"  and  seemed  to  remember  the  whole  visit 
pretty  clearly. 

A  curious  illustration  of  the  sense  of  epochal  lapse  of  time  that  a 
night's  sleep  probably  gave  her  was  given  after  she  had  had  a  heavy 
fall  and  bruised  her  lip  terribly.  She  had  been  assured  during  the 
first  day  that  if  she  was  patient  it  would  be  better  by  and  by:  the 
next  morning  on  waking,  she  put  up  her  finger  to  feel  the  lip,  and 
broke  into  a  disappointed  wail:  "It  's  sore  yet,  mamma!  " 

It  is  probable  that  her  own  attention  to  the  idea  of  sleep  began 
quite  early,  and  that  she  knew  in  some  sort  what  was  to  come  when 
she  first  began  to  show  resistance  to  being  put  to  sleep.  Her  con- 
sciousness of  it,  of  course,  must  have  been  limited  to  the  incipient 
stages  and  the  waking,  like  any  one  else's.  The  directions  "go 
bylow,"  and  "  go  to  sleep  "  were  obeyed  in  the  thirteenth  month  by 
laying  her  head  down  as  if  pretending  to  go  to  sleep:  and  from  the 
twelfth  month  to  the  eighteenth  she  was  devoted  to  playing  some- 
thing that  appeared  like  a  pretense  at  going  to  sleep,  —  laying  down 
her  head  on  a  cushion  or  pillow,  or  perhaps  lying  down  bodily  on 
the  floor,  to  pop  up  laughing;  by  the  fifteenth  month  shewouldcarry 
a  cushion  about,  and  put  it  on  the  floor  here  and  there  to  lie  down 
on.  In  the  fifteenth  month,  if  she  was  told  to  "sing,"  she  would 
utter  a  little  crooning  note  or  two,  and  put  down  her  head,  as  if 
she  were  being  lulled,  —  singing  being  associated  principally  with 
lullabies. 


294  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Toward  the  end  of  this  month  she  began  to  say  "By-y!"  when 
she  saw  the  rubber  nipple  she  went  to  sleep  with;  and  in  a  k\v  days 
it  became  evident  that  the  word  meant  sleep  or  lie  down  in  quite  a 
general  way:  e.  g.,  as  her  doll  lay  on  the  cushion,  —  "What  is  dolly 
doing?"  —  "By-y,"  —  she  is  asleep.  In  the  first  week  of  the  sixteenth 
month,  she  said,"Shlee-ee! "  on  seeing  some  one  yawn.  In  the  seven- 
teenth month,  as  she  trotted  about  with  her  favorite  play  of  lying 
down  on  cushion  or  floor,  she  would  say,  "By-y!"  and  "shee-ee!" 
(bylow,  sleepy)  as  she  laid  herself  down.  Once  in  this  month  (489th 
day)  she  said  of  an  invalid  who  lay  on  the  lounge  all  day  with  his 
books,  "G'ee'  —  by  —  boo',"  (Mr.  Greene  lies  down  and  has  books). 
Again,  seventy-third  week,  seeing  the  dog  lying  still,  she  observed, 
"  By ! "  In  the  eighteenth  month  by  and  sliee  were  used  interchange- 
ably for  sleep ;  but  on  the  whole  by  was  the  regular  word,  and  s/iee 
oftener  meant  sleepy;  by  would  be  used  if  she  saw  the  cat  or  dog 
lying  asleep.  At  twenty  months  I  note,  "Aunty  wake  up,"  "  Cousin 
little  Isabel  wake  up  too."  In  the  same  week  I  asked  her,  "  Where 
is  that  nap  Ruth  ought  to  have  had?  "  At  first  she  did  not  under- 
stand, but  when  I  repeated,  "That  nap, — -when  Ruth  shuts  her 
eyes  and  goes  to  sleep,  —  where  is  that  nap  ?  "  she  answered,  "  Gogn 
way."  —  "Why,  where  has  it  gone?"  —  "I'  be'"  (In  bed),  —  and 
in  this  answer  she  persisted,  declining  suggestions  that  she  should 
go  to  bed  to  find  it.  In  the  twenty-first  month  she  used  ''Wake  up" 
with  a  clear  distinction  between  transitive  and  intransitive,  "Wa'  u' 
owgu!"  (Let  us  go  and  wake  up  aunty);  "  Owgu  wa'  u'!  "  (Aunty  has 
waked  up). 

As  late  as  the  seventh  month,  she  did  not  shut  her  eyes  in  play- 
ing at  bylow:  but  in  the  twenty-first,  asked  what  eyes  were  for,  after 
once  answering  that  they  were  to  look,  she  said  a  second  time  that 
they  were  to  "go  by,"  —  to  go  to  sleep.  In  the  twenty-fourth 
month,  when  her  doll's  eyes  fell  out  (or  in)  her  only  comment,  her 
mother  told  me,  was,  "Dolly  can't  go  bylow."  In  the  twenty- 
seventh  month,  coming  to  me  with  her  doll  in  her  arms,  she  said 
suddenly,  "Aunty,  Bluebell  does  n't  go  to  sleep."  ■ — ■  "What,  not  at 
night?"  —  "  No."  —  'And  does  n't  little  Jenny?  "  [her  other  doll]  — 
"  No,"  —  very  positively,  shaking  her  head.  "  Why,  the  birdies  go 
to  sleep !  "  I  said,  intending  to  lead  her  to  speculate  on  the  difference 
between  animate  and  inanimate  things.     "  Yes."  —  "And  Ruth  goes 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  295 

to  sleep."  —  "Yes."  She  seemed  puzzled,  but  easily  left  the  subject. 
It  is  probable  that  she  understood  by  "sleeping"  nothing  more  than 
closing  the  eyes  :  indeed,  after  the  close  of  the  third  year,  I  saw 
some  strong  indications  that  this  was  so.  She  had  thus  far  had  no 
experience  of  dolls  that  could  shut  their  eyes. 

Not  to  separate  the  subject  of  Dreaming  from  that  of  Sleep,  I 
add  here  the  few  notes  I  was  able  to  make  concerning  Dreams.  I 
was  not  disposed  to  believe  that  the  child  dreamed  early,  and  did 
not  attribute  such  actions  as  sucking  in  her  sleep  (which  I  saw  in 
the  fourth  month)  nor  the  waking  and  crying,  nor  even  some  whim- 
pering in  sleep,  to  dreams,  since  it  was  quite  possible  that  all  these 
things  were  direct  results  of  sensory  conditions,  without  any  cerebral 
action.  The  first  distinct  evidence  of  dreaming  seen  by  either  the 
mother  or  myself  was  in  the  last  week  of  the  fourteenth  month 
(423d  day),  when  the  child  called  in  her  sleep,  "Owgng!  owgng!  " 
(Aunty).  Again  in  the  eighteenth  month  (535th  day),  the  mother 
told  me  that  the  child,  waking  in  the  night,  had  immediately  asked, 
"Do!"  (Outdoors),  —  a  request  hardly  comprehensible  under  the 
circumstances  unless  suggested  by  a  dream. 

In  the  first  half  of  the  third  year  I  saw  no  evidence  of  dreaming, 
but  her  mother  said  she  undoubtedly  dreamed  more  or  less :  in  the 
thirtieth  month  she  told  me  that  the  child,  waking  in  the  night,  had 
said,  "Mamma,  Aunty  May's  little  baby  can  walk!"  —  "Yes,  darling, 
by  and  by  she  can  walk."  —  "No,  mamma,  she  can  walk  now." 
Other  instances  were  also  given  me,  which  I  failed  to  record. 

In  the  thirty-second  month,  as  the  child  slept  beside  me,  I  heard 
her  speak  several  words  clearly  (I  did  not  catch  what  they  were), 
and  leaned  from  my  bed  to  see  what  she  wanted,  but  found  she  was 
asleep;  and  as  I  leaned  down,  she  laughed  aloud.  The  next  night, 
when  to  all  appearance  asleep,  she  suddenly  said  distinctly,  "Pi-ta- 
toes,"  ■ — a  pause,  then,  "No,  not  pitatoes,  pe-ta-toes."  Here  was 
clearly  dreaming,  but  whether  it  was  rare  or  customary,  I  could 
form  no  idea,  nor  whether  she  remembered  and  believed  in  the 
dreams. 

At  some  time  within  a  week  after  this  must  have  occurred  a 
dream  that  gave  light  on  the  last  point,  — ■  a  dream  about  a  bull,  of 
which  she  had  become  slightly  afraid.     She  murmured  something 


296  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i- 

about  him  in  the  night;  and  when  next  she  came  to  my  room  to 
sleep  (thirty-third  month),  she  asked,  "  Will  that  old  bull  come  here 
to-night,  aunty?"  When  I  assured  her  that  bulls  never  came  into 
the  house,  she  insisted  that  once  he  did.  She  talked  in  her  sleep 
that  night,  but  I  could  not  catch  the  words.  In  the  thirty-fifth 
month  she  still  insisted  that  the  bull  came  one  night  to  that  room, 
and  had  it  a  good  deal  on  her  mind  in  going  to  bed  the  first  night 
of  each  sojourn  with  me.  Indeed,  as  late  as  the  thirty- seventh 
month  she  still  recurred  to  it,  saying,  "Once  there  was  an  old  bull 
up  here,  aunty,"  but  was  now  easily  shaken  in  the  idea.  I  tried  to 
explain  to  her  something  of  what  a  dream  was,  but  I  have  no  idea 
that  she  understood  at  all.  I  never  saw  her  show  any  curiosity 
about  her  dreams.  That  of  the  bull  was  probably  remembered  only 
because  it  blended  with  certain  waking  memories,  merely  distorting 
them  a  little  (it  is  likely  that  without  such  backing  her  dreams  were 
too  filmy  and  fragmentary  for  memory):  the  bull  had,  in  fact, 
rushed  up  on  the  veranda,  trying  to  escape  from  the  men  that  were 
leading  him.' 

Dreaming  in  the  4th-6tii  Years. 

I  append  here  my  notes  on  Dreams  during  the  next  three  years: — 

One  night  in  the  37th  month  the  child  several  times  laughed  aloud  very 
merrily  in  her  sleep.  The  next  night  she  waked  and  asked,  "Has  my  mamma 
come  ?"  —  "Not  yet,"  I  said.  —  "But  I  saw  her  !  "  said  the  child;  and  repeated 
it  once  or  twice  in  a  puzzled  way.  The  next  morning  I  asked  her  about  it; 
did  she  think  she  saw  her  mamma  last  night? and  where?  "Coming  in  at  the 
door,"  she  said.  It  was  evident  that  she  was  not  romancing  at  all,  and  was 
still  unable  to  distinguish  between  the  dream  and  reality. 

About  two  weeks  later,  in  the  38th  month,  the  mother  told  me  that  waking 
from  sleep  the  child  had  said  to  her,  "I  killed  it,  mamma  !  "  —  "Killed  what?" 
—  "De  bad  sing.  I  killed  it  wiv  dis,"  holding  up  her  little  empty  hand;  then 
dropped  off  to  sleep  again.  She  showed  no  sign  of  fear.  Early  in  the  41st 
month,  however,  she  waked  in  the  night  and  scrambled  desperately  into  bed 
with  her  mother,  and  refused  to  be  put  back  into  her  crib,  saying  there  were 
bad  men  in  the  room,  —  papa  said  so.  She  scrambled  over  to  her  father  and 
clung  tightly  to  him.  In  the  morning  she  had  forgotten  all  about  it,  and  was 
sunny  and  bold  as  ever. 

In  the  48th  month,  waking  in  the  night,  she  asked,  "Ma  !  —  How  do  you 
think  Thad  kills  mice?"  —  "How?"  —  "  He  pulls  their  teeth  out."   A  pause, 

1  The  first  indication  of  dreaming  in  the  case  of  Mrs.  Chapman's  boy  was  in 
the  latter  part  of  the  12th  month,  when  toward  morning  he  rolled  over  in  a 
light  sleep,  murmuring,  "boom,  boom,"  (book).  He  then  settled  down  in  a 
deeper  sleep. 


Shinn.1  The  Development  of  a   Child.  297 

then,  "Once  Margie  and  I  saw  a  Jap  kill  a  mouse  that  way."  —  "No,  you 
did  n't,"  said  her  mother.  —  "Yes,  we  did/"  insisted  the  child.  —  "  No,  you 
^ever  saw  any  one  kill  a  mouse  that  way."  —  "No,  not  that  way,  but  we  saw  a 
Jap  kill  a  mouse  by  stepping  on  it."  After  a  pause:  "  Mice  do  lots  of  harmness, 
don't  they,  ma  ?" 

Here  was  probably  a  combination  of  dream  and  distorted  memory.  It  is 
likely  that  her  playmate,  Margie,  had  told  her  of  the  killing  of  the  mouse  by 
the  Japanese;  I  could  not  find  that  she  had  ever  had  any  opportunity  to  see 
anything  of  the  sort;  and  there  was  no  Japanese  workman  on  the  farm.  In  all 
these  dreams  of  the  fourth  year  the  influence  of  waking  experience  was  more 
or  less  traceable.  The  gruesome  nature  of  some  of  them  corresponded  to 
some  timidity  that  during  this  year  developed  in  waking  hours,  beginning  with 
her  fear  of  the  bull  at  three  years.  She  did  not  yet  seem  to  understand  the 
illusory  nature  of  dreams,  though  they  vanished  readily  from  her  memory  with 
daylight,  with  the  exception  of  the  one  about  the  bull. 

Early  in  the  fifth  year,  however,  she  began  to  tell  of  her  dreams,  and  in  a 
way  that  showed  she  understood  them  to  be  illusory.  Those  she  told  were 
quite  commonplace:  e.  g.,  she  dreamed  she  saw  an  old  woman  driving  a 
turkey;  she  dreamed  she  saw  the  cars  go  by,  right  along  our  carriage  drive,  — 
an  incongruity  which  she  laughed  at;  she  dreamed  she  saw  a  gull  fly  into  the 
cabin  of  the  ferry-boat.  She  had  often  seen  the  gulls  flying  around  the  boat. 
These  dreams  were  all  in  the  50th  month;  and  in  the  last  week  of  the  month 
she  dreamed  she  had  a  trumpet,  and  aunty  took  it  away  from  her  and  put  the 
end  in  the  fire;  she  began  to  cry,  and  aunty  took  it  out  and  showed  her  it  was 
not  hurt,  —  it  was  only  a  joke.  Where  she  got  the  idea  of  an  unkind  joke  I 
could  not  tell,  —  certainly  from  nothing  in  her  own  experience. 

In  the  53d  month  she  told  us  that  she  dreamed  some  soldiers  came,  and 
they  were  going  to  kill  her,  because  they  said  she  was  going  to  steal  something. 
"And  I  said,  '  I  don't  want  to  take  any  of  the  dried  fruit,'  so  they  let  me  go." 
She  had  been  at  the  fruit-drying  establishment  the  day  before,  and  had  asked 
for  some  fruit:  possibly  some  thought  of  less  lawful  means  of  obtaining  it  had 
crossed  her  mind  and  colored  her  dream.  During  the  great  railroad  strike, 
half  a  year  before,  she  had  seen  squads  of  soldiers  sent  out  to  keep  the  peace, 
and  had  stood  in  some  fear  of  them.  She  seemed  troubled  by  this  dream,  and 
complained  a  week  later  that  she  kept  thinking  of  it. 

In  the  54th  month  she  related  that  she  dreamed  there  was  a  pen  full  of 
squashes,  and  she  was  looking  over  at  the  squashes,  and  she  saw  a  big  black 
snake  among  them,  — just  the  tip  of  his  tail  sticking  out;  and  she  knew  if  she 
got  on  his  back  and  kicked  him  with  her  heels,  he  would  give  her  a  ride;  so 
she  got  on  his  back  and  kicked  him,  and  he  gave  her  a  ride,  clear  up  to  the 
cherry  trees.  I  suspect  that  Mowgli's  friend,  Kaa,  must  have  been  the  original 
of  this  accommodating  snake,  but  cannot  identify  exactly  enough  the  date  at 
which  she  made  Kaa's  acquaintance  to  connect  him  definitely  with  the  dream. 
Her  first  snake  acquaintance,  Nag,  in  Rikki-Tikki-Tavi,  was  not  at  all  likely  to 
inspire  such  a  dream. 

When  a  little  over  five  and  a  half  years  old  (67  months)  she  waked  whimper- 
ing a  little,  said  she  had  had  a  bad  dream,  and  asked  to  sleep  with  me  till 


298  University  of  California.  [Vol  i. 

morning:  she  said  if  she  got  back  into  her  own  bed  she  would  think  about  it. 
In  the  morning  I  asked  her  about  the  dream.  She  said  she  dreamed  there 
were  some  green  ants  with  a  kind  of  clubs,  and  one  of  them  hit  mamma's  head 
and  knocked  it  off;  then  she  [the  child]  begged  them  not  to  kill  her,  and  they 
did  n't;  and  she  begged  them  to  put  mamma's  head  on  again,  and  at  last  they 
did,  and  mamma  was  all  right. 

This  dream  seemed  so  probably  due  to  fairy  tales  that  she  was  told  we  must 
have  no  more  except  gentle  and  pleasant  tales  at  bedtime.  The  child's  firm 
nerves  and  unexcitable  imagination  had  led  to  a  good  deal  of  carelessness  in 
this  matter,  and  for  months  she  had  had  such  stories  as  she  asked  for  —  often 
Grimm's  tales  —  at  the  bedtime  hour,  without  showing  any  ill  effect  till  now. 
The  change  to  milder  ones  effected,  she  twice  again  dreamed:  once  something 
commonplace,  which  escaped  my  memory  before  I  could  record  it;  and  once 
that  there  was  a  pond  in  the  garden,  and  she  and  mamma  went  out  on  a  tongue 
of  land  that  extended  into  it,  and  picked  flowers  happily.  This  dream  was 
quite  accordant  in  spirit  with  the  stories  I  had  been  telling  her  at  bedtime. 

I  watched  her  dreaming  for  some  time,  but  except  on  these  two  occasions  she 
said  she  did  not  dream  at  all;  and  this  was,  as  far  as  I  could  ascertain,  the  rule. 
Up  to  seven  years  old,  she  insists  that  she  hardly  ever  dreams,  and  now  and 
then  mentions  having  had  a  dream  as  a  rare  and  interesting  occurrence.  Yet  I 
think  it  probable  that  some  drifting  images  occupy  her  sleep,  or  at  least  that 
her  consciousness  does  not  sink  below  the  level  at  which  lapse  of  time  can  be 
perceived;  for  once,  late  in  the  seventh  year,  she  was  greatly  surprised  on 
waking  without  having  had  this  sense  of  the  passage  of  time,  and  exclaimed 
over  and  over,  "I  thought  I  had  just  gone  to  sleep,  and  the  very  next  minute  I 
was  awake  and  it  was  morning  !  "  She  must  have  had  this  consciousness  of 
lapse  of  time  during  sleep  even  in  the  third  year  (p.  293,  above).  But  that 
dreaming  has  always  been  rare  I  feel  sure.  I  have  on  inquiry  been  told 
repeatedly  by  people  of  more  physical  than  mental  activity  that  they  scarcely 
ever  dreamed.  It  is  likely  that  in  primitive  times  dreaming  was  quite  rare, 
which  would  naturally  tend  to  enhance  its  mysterious  significance. 

I  was  well  satisfied  that  my  niece  never  at  any  time  invented  dreams;  nor 
after  she  was  four  years  old  did  she  ever  confuse  them,  that  I  could  detect, 
with  waking  thoughts  or  memories.  At  seven  years  old  she  can  even  dis- 
tinguish between  a  dream  and  waking  imagery  in  the  middle  of  the  night.  At 
this  age  she  waked  me  one  night  by  a  cry  that  brought  me  quickly  to  her  side. 
She  seemed  annoyed  and  abashed  when  she  found  me  by  her,  and  would  not 
tell  me  what  was  the  matter;  and  when  I  asked  if  it  was  a  bad  dream,  said  she 
did  not  think  so,  —  only  thoughts.  She  would  not  come  into  my  bed.  In  the 
morning,  however,  she  consented  to  explain:  it  was  black  cats,  she  said;  she 
kept  thinking  of  them,  as  if  the  air  was  full  of  them;  it  was  n't  a  dream,  only 
thoughts;  and  she  thought  perhaps  if  she  called  out,  it  would  make  them  go 
away.  A  more  imaginative  child,  doubtless,  would  have  experienced  far  more 
confusion  between  dreams,  subjective  imagery,  and  realities. 


SPONTANEOUS  MOVEMENTS. 

The  typical  movements  of  this  class  are  those  vague  and  un- 
co-ordinated  ones  which  appear  in  the  new-born  child  (indeed,  as 
Preyer  points  out,  they  are  only  continuations  of  intra-uterine 
movements),  and  which  seem  to  be  entirely  aimless,  unadaptive, 
and  independent  of  peripheral  stimulus.  Preyer  gives  them  the 
name  of  "Impulsive"  movements,  but  the  word  "Impulsive"  is 
used  by  English  psychological  writers  in  so  different  a  sense  that  I 
prefer  the  one  used  by  Bain,  and  adopted  by  Baldwin  and  Mrs. 
Moore,  viz.,  "Spontaneous."  The  description  of  these  movements 
given  by  Preyer  corresponds  with  my  own  observations.  He  attrib- 
utes them  to  random  jets  of  energy  disengaged  by  the  mere  growth 
of  the  lower  motor  centres  :  and  they  seem  to  me  of  such  character 
as  to  be  reasonably  accounted  for  by  this  theory.  Although  I  have 
called  them  entirely  unadaptive,  there  is  (as  Preyer  points  out)  one 
consistent  tendency  in  them,  viz.,  toward  the  resumption  of  the 
pre-natal  position :  but  this  does  not  in  any  way  conflict  with  his 
theory  of  their  origin,  since  the  motor  energy,  disengaged  without 
any  guidance,  would  naturally  find  oftenest  the  channels  already 
somewhat  worn,  so  to  speak. 

I  have  already  given,  under  the  head  of  Muscular  Sensation, 
(pp.  179,  180)  my  observations  concerning  the  purely  soontaneous 
movements,  and  the  manner  of  their  inhibition  by  more  adaptive 
ones.  By  the  end  of  the  first  three  months  I  had  ceased  to  see  any 
movements  that  I  thought  purely  spontaneous.1  There  were, 
however,  movements  of  a  mixed  or  transitional  character,  which 
are  best  classed  here. 

In  the  first  place,  I  observed  that  of  the  meaningless  movements 
made  by  the  baby  from  the  earliest  days,  some   appeared  to  be 

'Some  movements  that  might  perhaps  have  been  put  in  this  category,  I 
accounted  for  otherwise :  the  baby's  flourishing  of  her  rattle,  and  kicking  out 
her  legs  as  she  lay  on  her  mother's  lap,  I  thought  voluntary,  for  pure  pleasure 
in  muscular  activity;  wriggling  when  measured  (93d  day),  a  definite  expression 
of  discomfort;  the  vague  fumbling  movements  that  preceded  grasping,  primi- 
tive efforts  to  exercise  active  touch.  Preyer  thinks  many  of  the  early  vocal 
sounds  must  be  classed  as  "impulsive." 

(299) 


300  I  'niversity  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

connected  with  slight  variations  in  general  sensation,  and  therefore 
were  not  of  entirely  central  initiation.  The  facial  grimaces  and 
movements  of  the  head  seemed  to  be  stimulated  by  faint  discomfort, 
and  the  movements  of  the  arms  and  legs  by  heightened  general 
comfort.  During  the  first  months  the  irregular  movements  of  eyes 
and  limbs  were  always  increased  in  the  bath,  and  asymmetric  eye- 
movements  were  seen  there  after  they  had  ceased  under  other  con- 
ditions. These  irregular  muscular  contractions  and  relaxations  are 
not  to  be  taken  (as  Preyer  points  out)  for  true  expressions  of  joy 
or  discomfort,  although  the  latter  are  for  a  time  not  easily  dis- 
tinguished from  them.  After  the  first  six  weeks,  however,  the 
usual  expressive  movements  took  on  a  definite  and  recognizable 
character.  There  still  remained,  however,  instances  of  the  mixed 
or  transitional  class  of  movements.  Thus  in  the  sixth  week,  when 
the  baby  was  looking  with  most  intentness  at  anything,  she  would 
pant  in  short,  audible  breaths,  and  make  movements  with  her  hands, 
—  movements  quite  different  from  the  ordinary  flourishing  of  arms 
in  expression  of  joy  and  excitement,  and  less  co-ordinated  than  this; 
I  saw  these  vaguer  movements  as  late  as  the  8oth  day.  In  the 
seventh  week,  when  in  the  bath,  she  moved  her  head  from  side  to 
side  all  the  time  as  she  held  it  up,  breathed  rapidly,  and  pushed  her 
feet  against  the  foot  of  the  tub;  and  while  wiped  made  articulate 
noises,  panting,  her  head  turning  from  side  to  side,  and  arms  and 
legs  moving  gently.  The  47th  day  I  took  her  up  when  she  was 
crying  for  hunger,  and  noticed  that  she  was  doubling  up  and  stiffen- 
ing her  body,  —  a  movement  that  I  never  noticed  at  any  other 
time.  All  these  movements  (excepting  the  pressure  of  the  feet 
against  the  tub  and  perhaps  the  doubling  up  of  the  body),  seem 
to  have  been  due  to  an  irregular  overflow  of  general  stimulus 
from  sensation,  and  differed  from  several  of  the  ordinary  ex- 
pressive movements  only  because  the  latter  were  better  co- 
ordinated, and  were  regularly  associated,  each  with  its  usual 
stimulus  of  feeling.1  Such  irregular  expressions  of  feeling  —  over- 
flows, so  to  speak,  above  the  normal  methods  of  expression  — 
continue  even  in  adult  life:  in  extreme  pain  or  mental  distress,  e.  g., 

1  In  this  regular  association  of  the  movement  with  the  feeling,  not  only  in 
the  same  infant  at  different  times,  but  in  different  infants,  we  see  evidence  of 
an  inherited,  instinctive  character  even  in  very  simple  expressive  movements. 


Shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  301 

all  sorts  of  random  movements  —  tossing  the  hand  about,  twisting 
the  trunk,  etc.  —  are  resorted  to  by  excitable  persons  to  carry  off 
the  pressure  on  the  nerve-centers.  So  too  in  annoyance,  im- 
patience, sometimes  even  in  delight,  various  slight  movements, 
as  tapping  with  the  feet,  twisting  the  fingers,  etc.,  are  used  for 
like  purpose.  The  variety  of  movements  used  by  the  little  child 
to  work  off  a  high  pressure  of  spirits  is  illustrated  in  some  of 
my  notes  under  General  Sensation  (pp.  240,  242,  251).  Such 
movements  in  adult  life  are  to  a  certain  extent  conscious  and  volun- 
tary, but  not  always;  and  they  are  never  entirely  uncoordinated, 
for  with  the  increased  definiteness  of  nerve-channels  by  this  time, 
it  would  be  impossible  that  the  most  random  jets  of  energy  should 
produce  altogether  uncoordinated  movement  Even  at  two  or 
three  years  old,  and  in  sleep,  the  tossing  and  tumbling  of  a  child 
(movements  that  are  to  a  considerable  extent  spontaneous,  though 
doubtless  stimulated  more  or  less  by  pressure  of  bedclothes,  fatigue 
in  one  position,  etc.)  are  more  co-ordinated  than  the  ordinary 
waking  movements  of  a  baby  in  the  first  two  or  three  weeks:  the 
accustomed  groups  of  muscles  work  together  in  turning  the  body 
over,  for  instance,  —  a  co-operation  that  never  by  any  chance 
comes  about  in  the  earliest  weeks. 

Movements  resulting  from  an  excess  of  voluntary  motor  stim- 
ulus above  what  is  necessary  for  the  accomplishment  of  the  action 
willed,  so  that  a  secondary  action  (involuntary  and  unadaptive) 
results,  are  a  different  class  of  the  same  general  type  as  those 
just  considered.  Preyer  calls  them  "accompanying"  movements. 
Some  curious  jerkings  and  turnings  of  the  head  in  the  ninth  week, 
associated  with  the  effort  to  balance  it.  might  have  been  of  this 
class  (see  p.  327,  note);  the  wild  fluttering  of  the  hands  in  sympathy 
when  the  baby  was  trying  to  reach  an  object  with  her  lips  (Grasp- 
ing, p.  316)  is  a  good  instance.1 

1  Accompanying  movements  in  which  there  is  any  sort  of  adaptation,  even 
of  an  irrational  sort,  as  when  a  singer  lifts  the  brows  in  reaching  a  high  note, 
do  not  seem  to  me  properly  to  be  classed  here.  Such  are  the  movements  of 
keeping  time  to  music,  or  crooning  in  listening  to  it  (pp.  123-4),  and  the  sympa- 
thetic doubling  up  of  the  body  when  a  dive  was  made  with  the  head  for  the 
purpose  of  seizing  (p.  316);  a  similar  doubling  up  of  the  body,  also,  which 
occurred  when  the  baby  was  amusing  herself  by  making  vocal  sounds,  in  the 
twentieth  week,  —  she   would  suddenly  jump  at  a  sound,  so  to  speak.     It  is 


302  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

not  always  easy  to  determine  whether  an  accompanying  movement  is  due 
to  a  mere  blind  overflow  of  motor  stimulus,  or  whether  it  is  connected  with 
the  primary  movement  by  consistent  associations :  but  in  proportion  as  they 
are  found  habitually  occurring  together  it  must  be  concluded  that  the  con- 
nection is  not  a  random  one,  and  the  movement  is  not  properly  to  be  called 
spontaneous.  This  distinction  is  parallel  to  the  one  I  have  suggested  above 
between  movements  that  are  consistently  associated  as  expressions  with  certain 
states  of  feeling,  and  those  that  merely  express  vaguely  a  heightened  general 
excitability  in  the  motor  centers. 


REFLEX  MOVEMENTS. 

Under  this  head  Professor  Preyer  classes  all  movements  that 
follow  immediately  upon  peripheral  sensation  without  the  necessity 
of  any  central  action,  and  along  well-established  paths.  I  made  no 
especial  effort  to  keep  record  of  such  movements,  believing  that  to 
do  so  intelligently  required  more  special  physiological  knowledge 
than  I  had.  A  few  notes  of  the  common  reflexes  were  taken 
incidentally.  Sneezing  occurred  at  birth,  and  crying  soon  after; 
swallowing  also  on  the  first  day;  starting  (at  a  sound)  on  the  3d  or 
4th  day;  winking  on  the  23d;  hiccoughing  on  the  28th;  coughing 
on  the  42d.  Yawning  also  is  mentioned  on  the  43d  day,  but  with 
the  implication  that  it  had  been  seen  earlier:  on  this  day,  waking 
from  a  long  nap,  the  baby  yawned,  stretched,  and  seemed  to  be 
rubbing  her  fists  into  her  eyes,  in  a  comically  grown-up  manner. 
Choking  (in  nursing)  appeared  early,  but  I  have  not  the  exact 
date,  —  certainly  by  the  middle  of  the  third  month.  Closing  the 
hand  on  an  object  laid  in  the  palm  was  also  very  early,  —  I  believe 
from  the  first,  though  my  record  does  not  show  it. 

All  these  movements  were  as  complete  and  co-ordinated  as 
with  any  adult,  the  first  time  they  were  performed:  yet  it  cannot 
be  said  in  the  case  of  all  that  the  mechanism  seemed  to  be  at  birth 
in  perfect  order  and  ready  to  work  at  the  touch  of  the  appropri- 
ate stimulus.  Sneezing,  swallowing,  crying,  starting,  and  perhaps 
choking,  were  the  only  ones  of  which  this  could  be  said ;  and  even 
crying  was  not  the  more  complex  performance  of  later  infancy, 
but  a  mere  monotonous,  repeated  sound,  rather  a  bleat  than  true 
human  crying.  Winking  could  not  be  induced  in  the  first  three 
weeks:  sudden  sounds  caused  either  no  reaction  at  all  or  a  general 
start  throughout  the  body,  but  never  winking;  in  strong  light  the 
eyes  were  held  tightly  closed,  but  there  was  no  winking;  and  as 
already  noticed,  a  threat  at  the  eyes  produced  no  movement  of  the 
lids  before  the  56th  day.  This  was  of  course  partly  due  to  the 
imperfect  condition  of  sight  and  hearing,  so  that  the  proper  sen- 
sory stimulus  was  not  yet  applied;  but  the  reflex  mechanism  itself 
was  also  deficient,  for  the  earliest  winking  responses  were  usually 


304  University  of  California.  [Vol.  u 

remarkably  slow.  The  variability  of  this  response  (as  well  as  that 
of  starting)  after  sound  stimuli  was  probably  due  to  variable  sensi- 
bility to  sounds,  not  to  defects  in  the  reflex  mechanism  (Hearing, 
pp.  107-8);  when  first  seen  in  connection  with  visual  stimuli  the 
wink  followed  regularly,  though  slowly.  In  the  case  of  hiccough- 
ing, coughing,  and  yawning,  also,  it  seems  probable  that  the  condi- 
tions which  caused  the  movements  did  not  occur  for  the  first  time 
when  the  reactions  first  appeared. 

The  yawn  in  the  second  month  was  always  ended  with  a  com- 
ical little  throat-scraping  sound,  which  I  do  not  remember  to  have 
heard  from  any  older  person  in  yawning. 

Early  in  the  fourth  month  the  baby  became  able  to  produce  a 
cough  voluntarily,  but  not  by  imitation  of  the  natural  cough;  it 
seemed  to  be  merely  a  sound  hit  upon  independently  in  the  course 
of  experiments  with  the  vocal  organs. 

The  movement  of  rubbing  the  fists  in  the  eyes,  which  I  noticed 
(together  with  stretching  and  yawning,  see  above)  as  early  as  the 
seventh  week,  is  noteworthy:  for  I  am  certain  that  with  this  excep- 
tion the  hand  was  never  in  the  early  months  carried  to  the  spot  of 
any  peripheral  sensation.  It  must  have  been  a  purely  reflex  move- 
ment, for  it  was  not  before  the  tenth  week  that  the  hands  were 
voluntarily  guided  to  the  mouth;  and  it  seems  odd  that  so  unim- 
portant reflexes  as  these  should  appear  so  early  in  a  perfectly 
developed  and  remarkably  complex  group.  The  perfection  and 
maturity  of  the  movements  were  noticeable  to  the  most  casual 
observation,  and  had,  as  I  have  intimated,  a  comical  precocity  of 
appearance  in  so  tiny  a  baby,  —  out  of  keeping  with  the  rest  of  her 
behavior.  Rubbing  her  eyes  was  repeatedly  noticed  afterward,  but 
not  stretching. 

Other  and  less  habitual  reflex  movements  in  the  early  months, 
have  been  noted  under  other  heads,  but  may  be  repeated  here.1 
On  the  23d  day  the  arm  was  once  flung  out  at  a  sudden  sound  (the 
regular  response  being  a  wink  or  start);  and  in  the  last  week  of 
the  third  month,  when  I  kissed  the  baby's  hand  suddenly,  the  little 
hand  flew  up  as  if  a  spring  had  been  set  off.  Possibly  the  earliest 
smile  for  a  specific  cause  was  a  reflex  (it  was  excited  by  rubbing 

1  The  remarkable  turning  of  the  head  toward  an  interruption  of  the  light,, 
from  the  first  day,  is  considered  in  the  discussion  at  the  end  of  these  notes. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  T>°5 

the  upper  lip),  but  I  thought  that  it  merely  expressed  pleasure,  as 
smiles  noted  in  the  next  two  days  undoubtedly  did;  it  certainly 
was  not  a  reaction  following  "along  a  well-established  path."  The 
grimace  excited  by  novel  sensations  of  taste  (p.  160),  was  also  an 
expression  of  emotion  —  that  of  surprise  —  but  had  much  of  the 
quality  of  a  reflex.  It  is  hard  to  say  whether  pushing  with  the 
feet  against  a  surface  that  touches  the  soles  (pp.  180,  189)  is  to  be 
regarded  as  a  reflex  or  a  voluntary  movement.  The  121st  day, 
when  the  baby  was  listening  to  music,  and  already  in  an  excited 
nervous  condition,  I  could  feel  her  start  and  thrill  in  my  arms 
every  time  a  strong  chord  was  struck  or  a  vocal  note  accented 
heavily. 

Reflexes  were  quite  readily  inhibited  by  diversion  of  attention: 
see  under  Hearing  (p.  107)  the  failure  to  react  to  the  stroke  of  a 
bell  when  nursing,  23d  day;  and  the  power  of  the  piano  and  other 
diversions  to  check  crying  (pp.  108,  1 15,  149),  from  the  second 
month  on.  Even  sneezing  could  be  thus  checked,  from  the  eleventh 
month:  this  was  first  seen  on  the  316th  day,  when  some  one  mim- 
icked the  baby  as  she  was  about  to  sneeze,  and  made  her  stop  and 
smile,  losing  the  sneeze,  exactly  as  it  might  have  happened  with  a 
grown  person.  In  this  case,  however,  the  diversion  of  attention 
may  not  have  had  so  much  to  do  with  checking  the  sneeze  as  the 
movement  of  the  lips  in  smiling. 


INSTINCTIVE  MOVEMENTS.1 
Grasping. 

From  the  first  the  baby  would  close  her  fingers,  like  all  babies, 
on  any  small  object  laid  in  her  palm;  and  the  tendency  of  her 
hands  to  move  upward,  about  her  head  and  face  (see  p.  299), 
looked  somewhat  like  an  effort  to  reach  her  mouth;  but  these  early 
movements  must  be  set  down  as  quite  involuntary,  as  the  later 
slow  development  of  the  same  acts  under  volitional  control  shows. 
Neither  intentional  holding  of  an  object,  nor  intentional  seeking  of 
the  mouth  with  the  hands,  was  seen  before  the  third  month,  with 
the  remarkable  exception  about  to  be  related:  — 

On  the  48th  day,  just  after  nursing,  the  baby  lay  in  her  mother's 
lap  looking  about,  quiet  and  contented.  I  laid  a  pencil  some  three 
inches  long  in  her  hand,  intending  to  see  whether  there  was  yet 
any  indication  of  consciousness  and  purpose  in  holding.  The  hand 
closed  on  it  at  once  (the  thumb  correctly  reversed)  and  carried  the 
pencil  to  the  mouth.  I  had  no  idea  that  this  could  be  more  than 
accident,  but  pushed  the  hand  away  from  the  neighborhood  of  the 

1  I  am  quite  aware  of  the  difficulties  of  definition  that  surround  the  use  of 
the  word  Instinct,  and  the  uncertainty  of  the  line  between  instinctive  and  reflex 
actions  on  the  one  side,  and  instinctive  actions  and  personal  habits  on  the  other. 
It  certainly  seems  a  doubtful  classification  that  separates  a  simple  and  primitive 
act  like  sucking  from  another  simple  and  primitive  act  like  swallowing,  and 
puts  it  into  the  same  class  with  walking,  which  the  child  inherits  only  as  a 
tendency,  and  actually  learns  with  as  much  individual  effort  as  an  adult  employs 
in  learning  to  use  the  bicycle.  Sucking,  biting,  and  licking  plainly  resemble 
the  instincts  of  the  lower  animals,  as  the  pecking  of  newly-hatched  chickens, 
the  standing  of  the  new-born  calf :  they  are  inherited  ready-made.  The  loco- 
motor movements  and  grasping,  on  the  other  hand,  approach  the  character  of 
an  individual  acquirement  that  speaking  possesses,  —  the  one  step  that  sepa- 
rates them  being  the  necessity  of  a  model  of  language  to  imitate  before  speech 
can  be  acquired,  while  we  feel  sure  that  an  absolutely  isolated  child  would 
begin  to  grasp  and  to  walk  when  the  time  came.  Yet  this  one  step  is,  I  think, 
the  significant  one.  The  locomotor  movements  are  not  only  the  same  ones  — 
homologous  in  nature  —  with  those  that  we  recognize  as  instinctive  in  the  lower 
animals,  but  also  they  are  not  in  a  broad  sense  volitional  and  individual: 
though  individual  intelligence  and  volition  attend  their  acquisition  to  a  quite 
(306) 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  ^O'J 

face,  lest  the  pencil-point  should  do  harm  in  some  aimless  move- 
ment. To  my  surprise  the  baby  six  times  carried  the  pencil  directly 
back  as  I  pushed  it  away;  and  as  she  did  so  she  put  out  her  lips 
and  tongue  toward  it  eagerly,  with  sucking  motions,  much  as  when 
about  to  be  put  to  the  breast,  —  looking,  as  was  her  habit  when 
put  to  the  breast,  not  at  the  object  but  vaguely  before  her.  She 
held  the  pencil  firmly  for  about  three  minutes,  then  her  attention 
seemed  to  wander  from  it  and  her  fingers  relaxed.  I  could  not  get 
any  repetition  of  the  incident,  either  with  that  hand  or  the  other: 
her  fingers  would  close  on  it  as  always,  but  without  attention,  and 
would  soon  relax.  It  was  weeks  before  the  least  attempt  to  carry 
the  hands  to  the  mouth  could  be  detected  again.  Yet  it  seemed 
impossible  to  doubt  that  there  had  been  an  effort  to  do  so  on  this 
occasion.  I  could  only  suppose  that  the  first  upward  movement 
had  been  mere  chance,  but  that  the  hand  or  pencil-tip  had  barely 
touched  the  baby's  lips  before  I  pushed  it  away,  and  that  the  asso- 
ciation of  this  sensation  with  the  muscular  movement  of  the  arm 
lasted  long  enough  to  induce  the  half-dozen  immediate  repetitions 
of  the  movement,  but  not  long  enough  to  create  any  fixed  associa- 
tion group. 

It  is  evident  that  the  hand  as  an  organ  of  grasping  was  not 
concerned    in    this    incident,  except   by  the    inherited    reflex  that 

remarkable  extent,  yet  they  are  as  necessary  and  racial  in  the  child  as  in  the 
calf :  ever\r  normal  child  that  is  born  must  needs  come  to  them,  at  about  the 
same  age  and  in  somewhat  similar  manner.  They  have  therefore  the  essential 
characters  of  instinct,  as  agreed  upon  by  all  different  definitions  of  the  word. 
If  we  find  on  the  one  hand  very  marked  instinctive  elements  in  movements 
that  are  on  the  whole  classed  as  volitional,  and  find  on  the  other  hand  that 
such  an  action  as  sucking  or  biting  occupies  a  ground  almost  intermediate 
between  reflex  and  instinctive  action,  so  that  on  either  side  of  the  field  of 
Instinct  some  acts  might  be  cut  away  or  added,  according  to  the  exact  defi- 
nition that  one  uses,  this  only  shows  the  more  plainly  that  sharp  lines  of  di- 
vision are  not  made  here  by  nature,  —  that  the  different  types  of  race  move- 
ments shade  into  each  other.  Even  among  undoubtedly  reflex  actions,  which 
follow  directly  upon  peripheral  stimulus  without  any  interposition  of  volition, 
perhaps  not  even  of  consciousness,  according  to  fixed  ancestral  arcs  of  communi- 
cation, there  is  every  grade  of  volitional  infusion  into  the  act :  thus,  coughing 
and  swallowing  can  be  voluntarily  produced,  but  only  up  to  a  certain  point 
inhibited ;  yawning  cannot  be  produced  or  inhibited  by  direct  volition,  but 
is  notoriously  subject  to  suggestion,  while  choking  cannot  possibly  be  inhibited 
by  any  central  cause,  nor  under  ordinary  conditions  suggested. 

21 


308  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

enabled  it  to  hold  fast  the  pencil.  The  mouth,  however,  was 
already  considerably  developed  as  a  grasping  organ,  through  its 
primitive  function  of  sucking.  From  the  fourth  week  the  baby,  if 
held  against  one's  cheek  when  she  was  hungry,  would  lay  hold  on 
it  with  her  lips  and  suck  it.  Signs  of  especial  tactile  sensibility 
in  the  lips,  and  even  the  use  of  active  touch  in  the  tongue,  had 
already  been  noted  (Sense  of  Contact,  p.  136),  but  none  in  the 
fingers.  Blind  groping  movements  with  the  head  when  placed  at 
the  breast,  in  the  fifth  week,  showed  an  instinctive  disposition  to 
co-operation  of  the  neck  with  the  mouth  in  grasping. 

During  the  second  month  I  saw  further  evidence  of  special  tac- 
tile sensibility  about  the  lips  and  tongue,  in  the  baby's  habit  when 
not  hungry  of  putting  her  lips  to  one's  cheek,  if  she  was  laid 
against  it,  and  licking  it,  instead  of  sucking,  as  she  did  when 
hungry.  Meanwhile  no  special  sensibility,  even  passive,  appeared 
in  the  fingers  till  the  beginning  of  the  third  month,  when  the  baby 
had  a  habit  of  keeping  her  finger-tips  together  if  they  chanced  to 
come  into  contact,  as  though  interested  in  the  sensation. 

By  this  time  —  the  beginning  of  the  third  month  —  she  would 
keep  her  hands  closed  for  several  minutes  (the  thumb  as  well  as 
the  fingers)  on  a  finger  laid  in  the  palm,  though  not  on  any  other 
object.  This  seemed  more  like  conscious  holding,  yet  as  will  be 
seen  below  (p.  310)  it  was  not  till  late  in  the  month  that  I  saw 
clear  evidence  of  purpose  and  attention  in  holding. 

She  now  began  (ninth  week)  to  make  efforts  to  get  her  hands 
to  her  mouth.  By  the  tenth  week  the  fists  were  put  to  the  mouth 
constantly,  usually  both  at  once,  and  whatever  parts  of  them  could 
get  inside  were  sucked  and  mumbled.  If  they  were  taken  away 
the  baby  would  cany  them  back.  When  her  hands  closed  mechan- 
ically on  folds  of  her  clothes  or  of  the  towel  as  she  was  wiped, 
these  were  carried  along  with  her  hands  to  her  mouth  and  sucked; 
but  there  was  no  conscious  attempt  to  carry  objects  thither.  By 
the  twelfth  week  she  had  learned  to  get  her  thumb  into  her  mouth 
whenever  she  wished,  and  was  fond  of  sucking  it  as  she  lay  cozily 
against  her  mother's  shoulder.  If  she  lost  it  out,  she  usually  tried 
to  put  her  mouth  down  to  it,  instead  of  putting  it  up  to  her  mouth 


shinn]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  309 

When  she  had  been  diverted  from  the  thumb  for  a  little  while,  she 
would  recur  to  it  suddenly,  making  a  dive  of  her  head  for  it,  and  at 
the  same  time  carrying  it  upward  to  meet  her  mouth.  This  dive 
for  her  thumb  was  her  only  sudden  movement,  and  very  comical,  — 
as  if  she  had  ejaculated,  "Happy  thought,  —  to  suck  my  thumb!" 
The  continuance  of  the  movement  as  late  as  the  fifth  month  is 
noted  below  (p.  316). 

But  though  the  precedence  of  the  mouth  as  ,1  grasping  organ 
was  quite  evident  here,  the  hands  in  this  same  twelfth  week  came 
rapidly  to  the  front  in  consciousness,  and  by  the  end  of  the  week 
had  made  visible  progress  toward  grasping.  The  first  sign  of  this 
was  a  trick  of  clasping  and  unclasping  the  fingers  fumblingly  on 
some  surface,  as  one's  hand  or  dress,  evidently  with  intention,  — 
an  action  which  I  could  not  attribute  to  anything  but  the  exercise 
of  active  feeling,  hitherto  seen  only  in  lips  and  tongue.  Objects 
placed  in  the  hands  were  held  very  firmly,  though  not  looked  at. 
The  79th  day  a  large  rubber  ring,  placed  in  the  baby's  hand  and 
carried  with  it  to  her  mouth,  was  held  there  for  a  minute  or  two 
while  she  mumbled  and  sucked  at  it.  This  approached  to  con- 
scious holding.  The  next  day,  when  bright  napkin  rings  were 
rolled  across  the  tray  of  the  high-chair  in  which  the  baby  was 
propped,  she  looked  at  them  with  eagerness,  making  indefinite 
motions  with  her  hands,  which  might  have  had  some  sort  of  antici- 
patory connection  with  the  impulse  to  seize,  but  which  I  regarded 
as  merely  accompaniments  of  cerebral  tension,  since  such  motions 
were  not  infrequent  in  connection  with  intent  interest  (see  Spon- 
taneous Movement,  p.  300).  On  the  83d  day,  as  the  baby  sat  in' 
her  high-chair  with  rattles  and  other  articles  lying  on  the  tray  of 
the  chair,  I  noticed  that  she  kept  fumbling  on  the  tray  with  her 
hands,  and  that  when  she  touched  an  article  she  would  at  once 
grasp  and  raise  it,  sometimes  holding  it  for  half  a  minute,  but 
without  looking  at  it  or  the  hand,  or  showing  any  perception  that 
the  article  she  felt  in  her  hand  was  the  same  that  she  saw  when 
she  did  chance  to  giance  at  it.  In  picking  it  up,  if  it  happened  to 
touch  her  fingers  conveniently  in  front,  she  would  clasp  it  properly; 
but  I  noticed  once  that  when  the  back  of  her  hand  came  in  contact 
with  a  rattle  she  had  no  idea  of  turning  her  hand,  but  got  hold  of 
the  rattle  backward  between  two  fingers,  and  so  lifted  it. 


310  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Here  was  already  developed  suddenly  a  sort  of  hand-grasp- 
ing, —  the  ability  to  lay  hold  upon  an  object  when  it  was  felt  in 
contact  with  the  hand;  but  it  was  nearly  a  month  more  before 
grasping  at  a  visually  located  object  appeared.  In  the  interval 
steady  progress  was  made  in  skill  and  conscious  intention  in  laying 
hold  of  objects  and  raising  them,  and  I  watched  and  recorded  every 
step  of  this  progress  strictly,  feeling  that  it  marked  the  beginning 
of  an  important  epoch  of  development. 

For  some  three  weeks  this  progress  in  grasping  all  took  place 
when  the  baby  was  propped  in  her  high-chair  and  supplied  with 
convenient  objects  on  the  tray  of  the  chair,  where  she  was  doubtless 
already  learning  to  grope  for  them.  During  the  thirteenth  week  I 
find  almost  daily  note  of  more  skill  in  taking  hold.  On  the  S6th 
day  the  baby  repeatedly  picked  up  her  rattle,  which  had  a  slender 
shaft  easily  grasped;  and  I  noticed  that  when  fretting  a  moment  for 
some  reason  she  did  not  hold  the  rattle,  but  relaxed  her  grasp 
at  once,  showing  that  the  holding  was  now  conscious,  requiring 
attention.  I  noticed  also  that  when  holding  the  rattle  and  flourish- 
ing the  arm  she  no  longer  flourished  the  other  arm,  from  which  I 
thought  there  was  certainly  volition  in  the  action :  the  rattle  was 
no  longer  merely  held  while  the  arms  went  through  the  customary 
movements  of  joy  and  excitement  (in  which  both  arms  were  always 
used),  but  was  purposely  shaken.  There  may  even  have  been  an 
association  of  this  movement  with  the  jingling  sound,  and  a  desire 
to  produce  it.  Objects  were  not  yet  voluntarily  carried  to  the 
mouth ;  but  they  were  frequently  brought  up  into  its  neighborhood 
by  the  motions  of  the  arms,  and  once  on  this  86th  day,  when  the 
rattle  had  thus  come  into  the  neighborhood  of  the  mouth,  I  saw 
the  baby  try  to  put  it  in;  she  had  it  at  right  angles  to  the  mouth 
with  the  rubber  nipple  on  the  end  almost  in,  but  at  this  point  she 
was  interrupted.  The  next  day  she  not  only  took  up  the  rattle 
with  increased  skill  and  intention  when  her  fingers  touched  it,  and 
shook  it  as  before,  but  tried  unmistakably  and  repeatedly  to  carry 
it  to  her  mouth,  —  thus  repeating  for  the  first  time  the  effort  made 
with  the  pencil  on  the  48th  day  (p.  307).  The  shaft  was  so  long  that 
she  could  not  get  the  nipple  on  its  end  into  her  mouth  except  by 
holding  it  diagonally ;  yet  she  accomplished  it  several  times,  very 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  3  I  I 

clumsily.  She  fumbled  for  her  mouth,  sometimes  thrusting  the 
rattle  against  some  other  part  of  her  face,  thence  feeling  her  way 
along  with  it  gradually  to  the  right  place;  three  times  she  brought 
it  up  sidewise,  so  that  a  ring  of  little  bells  with  which  it  was  fringed 
touched  her  lips,  when  she  would  draw  the  little  bells  in  and  suck 
them.  Her  father  was  unable  to  resist  helping  her  by  placing  the 
rattle  in  her  mouth,  so  I  could  not  judge  what  she  would  have 
accomplished  if  let  alone.  The  difficulty  she  experienced  in  finding 
her  mouth  with  it,  compared  with  the  ease  and  directness  with 
which  she  could  put  her  thumb  in  the  desired  spot,  was  noticeable. 

Twice  on  first  grasping  the  rattle  she  looked  at  it  seriously  for 
some  time,  but  did  not  appear  to  connect  its  visual  appearance  with 
what  she  felt  in  her  hand. 

The  next  day  (88th)  she  took  up  the  rattle  readily  when  her  hand 
touched  it,  and  turning  the  rubber  end  (probably  by  chance)  toward 
her  mouth,  lifted  it;  it  struck  at  the  side  of  her  nose  about  an  inch 
above  her  mouth,  and  instead  of  moving  the  rattle  she  tried  to 
reach  it  by  stretching  her  mouth  wide  and  moving  her  head. 
After  a  few  vain  efforts  of  this  sort  she  lowered  her  hand  and  made 
a  fresh  start,  —  this  time  hitting  her  lips,  where  by  stretching  her 
mouth  again  she  managed  to  reach  the  rubber  and  get  it  into  her 
mouth,  but  awkwardly,  in  her  cheek.  Again  her  father  could  not 
resist  coming  to  her  aid,  so  I  could  not  tell  whether  she  would 
have  righted  it  herself.  After  a  little  she  lost  it,  then  grasped  it 
again  and  brought  it  to  her  mouth,  this  time  the  other  end  upper- 
most. This  end  had  instead  of  the  nipple  a  large  sleigh-bell, 
fringed  with  small  ones;  and  thinking  it  undesirable  for  her  to 
suck  the  little  bells,  her  mother  took  the  rattle  away  to  remove 
them,  and  the  baby  lost  interest  and  would  not  renew  her  efforts 
when  the  rattle  was  given  back. 

She  had  a  rubber  nipple  set  in  an  ivory  disk,  with  a  movable 
ivory  ring  on  the  other  side ;  this  nipple  she  did  not  try  to  put 
into  her  own  mouth,  but  held  and  sucked  it  when  it  was  put 
there  by  others.  This  afternoon,  having  had  it  put  into  her  mouth, 
she  presently,  in  putting  up  her  hand,  got  hold  of  the  ring,  grasped 
it  firmly,  and  soon  accidentally  pulled  out  the  nipple.  This  hap- 
pened repeatedly,  and  she  also  lost  it  out  in  trying  to  laugh  or 
shout  while  holding  it. 


312  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

During  the  rest  of  the  thirteenth  week  the  clumsy  and  rarely 
successful  attempts  to  get  the  rattle  into  her  mouth  continued. 
Once  when  she  brought  the  bell  end  to  her  mouth,  being  unable 
to  get  the  bell  in  as  she  did  the  nipple  at  the  other  end,  she 
began  licking  it. 

She  was  now  just  three  months  old.  Besides  the  power  to 
lay  hold  on  an  object  that  touched  her  fingers  and  to  carry  it 
imperfectly  to  her  mouth,  she  had  showed  some  voluntary  use  of 
hands  and  arms  in  the  effort  to  pull  herself  to  a  sitting  position 
when  holding  with  her  hands  to  some  one's  forefingers,  —  an 
advanced  movement  rather  difficult  to  understand  at  this  stage, 
and  apparently  quite  blindly  instinctive,  for  it  seems  improbable 
that  she  should  have  formed  any  association  between  this  pulling 
movement  and  the  raising  of  her  body;  still,  her  mother  had 
often  lifted  her  by  the  arms  in  bathing  and  wiping.  She  would 
hold  the  forefingers  tight  and  firm,  and  pull  hard  (see  p.  328).  This 
movement  had  been  made  for  some  weeks. 

I  now  watched  vigilantly  for  the  first  sign  of  any  visual  guid- 
ance in  seizing;  but  for  some  time  in  vain.  The  baby  continued 
to  touch  objects  either  by  accident,  or  perhaps  by  fumbling  for  them, 
looking  in  some  other  direction  inattentively;  but  the  object  once 
felt,  she  would  seize  it  with  clear  intention  and  carry  it  to  her 
mouth.  If  by  chance  her  eyes  did  turn  toward  the  object,'  it 
was  with  entire  inattention.  The  98th  day,  when  pounding  first 
with  a  rattle  and  then  with  her  hand,  with  much  violence  (p.  144), 
she  did  not  once  glance  at  the  rattle  or  hand;  a  few  days  before, 
when  I  kissed  her  hand  suddenly  and  apparently  startled  her, 
though  the  hand  flew  up  sharply  (p.  304)  she  did  not  look  at  it; 
about  the  same  time,  when  a  visitor  amused  her  greatly  by  kissing 
her  fingers  rapidly,  though  she  smiled  and  cooed  her  eyes  never 
turned  from  the  lady's  face  to  her  own  hand. 

On  the  99th  day  I  noticed  that  she  several  times  looked  down 
at  an  object  while  grasping,  but  never  before  she  had  laid  hold,  and 
never  with  any  appearance  of  attention.  In  the  following  week, 
(fifteenth)  she  quite  commonly  let  her  eyes  rest  rather  blankly  on 
her  hands  and  objects  in  them  after  she  had  fumbled  about  till  she 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  3r3 

touched  an  object  and  while  she  was  lifting  it ;  then  her  glance 
would  leave  it,  and  it  would  be  carried  to  her  mouth  by  feeling  only. 

By  this  time  her  skill  in  taking  things  up  had  considera- 
bly improved:  on  the  105th  day,  for  instance,  I  note  that 
having  brought  her  hands  against  a  napkin  ring  that  lay  be- 
fore her,  she  took  hold  of  it  neatly  with  both  hands,  one  on 
each  side,  and  tried  to  lift  it.  This  shows  a  certain  amount  of 
co-operation  between  the  hands:  yet  her  understanding  of  them 
and  their  relation  to  her  was  plainly  very  slight :  if  in  fumbling  for 
an  object  they  chanced  to  encounter  each  other,  each  would  grasp 
the  other  and  try  to  carry  it  to  the  mouth.  She  could  carry  a 
small  object,  as  a  rattle,  quite  easily  to  her  mouth  now ;  but  was 
still  embarrassed  in  getting  it  in  by  any  difficulties  in  its  shape; 
she  would  often  then  drop  the  object  and  resort  to  the  ever- 
convenient  thumb  or  fingers  instead,  with  the  same  droll  sudden- 
ness as  before.  Often  in  lifting  an  object  to  her  mouth  she  would 
open  her  mouth  in  readiness,  and  sometimes  would  bend  her 
head,  —  mouth  and  hands  seeking  for  each  other  mutually. 

In  the  sixteenth  week  her  disposition  to  put  things  into  her 
mouth  and  her  skill  in  getting  them  there  grew;  and  she  began  to 
take  hold  of  objects  elsewhere  than  on  her  tray:  she  would  clutch 
at  people's  fingers  or  at  the  folds  of  dresses  as  her  hands  touched 
them,  and  pull  them  to  her  mouth.  Yet  she  still  understood  so 
little  the  relation  of  her  arms  and  their  movements  to  the  presence 
of  objects  in  her  mouth,  that  she  constantly  lost  the  rattle  out  of  her 
mouth  by  trying  to  flourish  her  arms  while  she  held  and  sucked  it. 
She  even  lost  her  fingers  out  in  the  same  way. 

At  just  sixteen  weeks  old  (113th  day),  she  made  a  near 
approach  to  deliberate  grasping:  she  looked  at  her  mother's  hand 
held  out  to  her,  and  while  looking  made  fumbling  motions  toward 
it  with  her  own  hand  till  she  struck  it,  then  seized  it  and  tried  to 
cany  it  to  her  mouth;  and  twice  again  the  same  day  I  saw  her 
do  this.  She  would  not  aim  a  grasp  at  the  object,  under  visual 
guidance,  but  would  look  at  it,  move  her  hands  vaguely,  as  if 
feeling  for  it,  then  strike  them  toward  it  with  fingers  open  till  they 
touched;  then  she  would  seem  to  understand  what  she  was  about, 
and  take    hold.     She   looked    more   than   before    at    objects    held 


314  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

in  her  hand,  but  still  vaguely;  and  indeed,  the  whole  process  had 
a  vague  and  mechanical  appearance,  as  if  there  were  little  volition 
about  it.  The  114th  day  she  got  hold  of  a  good  many  objects  by 
a  kind  of  vague  clawing  at  them,  looking  at  them  more  and  more 
as  she  did  so. 

But  with  regard  to  putting  objects  once  seized  into  the  mouth, 
the  volition  was  clear:  as  far  back  as  the  96th  day  her  grandmother 
had  seen  her  open  her  mouth  while  getting  hold  of  her  rattle,  and 
now  it  was  common  ;  she  would  open  her  mouth  and  put  her  head 
forward  as  soon  as  she  touched  an  object,  even  while  fumbling  to 
get  a  good  hold,  —  but  never  at  sight  of  it.  The  112th  day  I 
touched  her  face,  and  she  made  many  efforts  to  reach  my  touching 
finger  with  her  mouth,  moving  her  head  to  help,  but  did  not  lift 
her  hand  toward  it. 

The  114th  day  I  tried  the  experiment  of  giving  her  a  rattle  in 
each  hand,  and  she  flourished  both  happily,  though  giving  most 
attention  to  the  one  she  had  first,  in  the  right  hand.  Later  the 
same  day,  seeing  her  in  high  content,  holding  one  in  her  left  hand, 
flourishing  and  chewing  it,  I  put  another  into  her  right  hand  :  this 
time  the  division  of  attention  seemed  to  trouble  her ;  she  made  a 
pitiful  face  and  began  to  fret,  and  was  not  happy  till  the  intruding 
rattle  was  removed. 

The  1 1 8th  day  occurred  the  incident  that  may  be  called  the 
first  real  grasping,  —  if  it  is  possible  to  fix  the  point  in  such  a 
gradual  development.  I  held  the  baby  up  before  a  picture  on  the 
wall,  which  she  was  accustomed  to  look  at  for  some  seconds  with 
interest.  The  light  shining  on  the  walnut  frame  seemed  to  catch 
her  eye :  she  looked  at  it,  put  out  her  hand  a  little  uncertainly  and 
waveringly,  and  first  touched  then  took  hold  of  the  edge  of  the 
frame.  1  then  brought  her  rattle  and  held  it  out  some  two  inches 
from  her  hand  :  she  put  out  her  hand  in  the  same  uncertain  way 
and  took  it.  In  the  afternoon  she  had  somewhat  relapsed  from 
this  attainment :  when  the  rattle  was  offered  her  she  looked  at  it, 
making  some  sounds  of  desire,  moved  her  arms  vaguely,  and 
finally  brought  both  hands  down  about  it,  on  either  side ;  as  soon 
as  she  touched  it  her  movements  became  definite,  and  she  laid  hold 
of  it  and  carried  it  skilfully  to  her  mouth.     This  was  the  way  that 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  315 

at  this  time  she  took  up  articles  from  her  tray,  —  bringing  her 
hands  down  to  them  from  either  side  with  much  uncertainty  till 
she  touched  them;  then  she  would  lift  napkin  ring  or  spool,  rattle 
or  rubber  doll,  quite  firmly  to  her  mouth,  often  with  both  hands. 
I  have  seen  her  hold  a  napkin  ring  very  nicely  at  this  time,  grasping 
the  opposite  edges  with  her  two  hands. 

The  next  day  there  was  no  advance  in  seizing  with  the  hands, 
but  two  instances  of  the  more  primitive  mouth-grasp.  The  mother 
tried  once  more  to  see  if  the  baby,  when  about  to  nurse,  could  find 
the  nipple  by  sight,  a  thing  she  had  never  been  able  to  do 
before;  she  found  it  now  with  lips  and  head  quite  easily,  at  a 
distance  of  some  three  inches,  looking  at  it  as  she  did  so.  In  her 
baby-carriage  she  got  her  hands  out  from  under  cover,  and  taking 
hold  of  a  fold  of  her  cloak,  put  it  into  her  mouth.  As  the  day  was 
cold  I  put  the  hands  back  under  cover ;  when  the  baby,  without 
trying  to  get  them  out,  put  out  her  tongue  and  made  efforts  to 
reach  the  cloak  with  it.  After  a  little  she  gave  this  up  and  got 
her  hands  out  again. 

The  1 20th  day  (seventeen  weeks  old)  I  showed  her  a  colored 
picture-book.  She  looked  at  it  with  interest  and  put  out  her  hands 
to  touch  the  pages,  feeling  till  she  got  hold  of  the  edge,  when  she 
at  once  pulled  the  book  to  her  mouth.  I  then  held  it  beyond  her 
reach,  and  she  stretched  out  both  arms  toward  it  as  far  as  she 
could.  Her  grasping  was  still,  however,  to  a  great  extent  feeling 
for  the  object,  without  much  dependence  on  visual  direction:  she 
would  put  out  her  hand  with  fingers  lightly  spread  or  curled,  not 
prepared  to  seize,  touch  the  object  with  the  tips,  then  feel  about 
it  till  she  got  a  good  hold. 

As  an  instance  of  her  skill  in  taking  up  an  object  from  her 
tray  at  the  end  of  the  fourth  month,  I  saw  her  on  the  I22d  day 
take  up  a  heavy  silver  napkin  ring  nicely  with  one  hand,  and  carry 
it  to  her  mouth. 

With  the  beginning  of  the  fifth  month  there  was  a  perceptible 
advance  in  precision  and  confidence  of  grasping,  yet  the  method 
remained  the  same.  I  watched  it  carefully:  the  baby  would  first 
put  her  hand  uncertainly  out  with  fingers  spread  and  no  appearance 
of  intention  to  grasp,  but  looking  at  the  object  always;  when  she 


3  1 6  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

had  once  touched  it  her  motion  would  become  more  confident; 
she  would  feel  about  it  till  she  had  a  good  grasp,  and  then  take  it. 
There  could  be  no  doubt  of  the  deliberate  character  of  the  action 
since  about  the  time  she  took  hold  of  the  picture  frame.  Once, 
the  130th  day,  her  grandfather  came  up  to  her  and  held  out  both 
forefingers,  and  she  lifted  her  hands  and  laid  hold  on  a  finger  with 
each  hand  quite  promptly,  but  could  not  be  got  to  repeat  the 
action.  She  liked  at  this  time  (nineteenth  week)  to  pull  her  father's 
and  grandfather's  whiskers:  they  had  begun  this  by  tangling  her 
hands  in  their  whiskers,  but  now  the  action  was  plainly  voluntary. 
She  did  not  thus  far  care  much  for  grasping,  her  interest  being 
centered  in  the  use  of  her  eyes  and  voice:  nor  did  she  show 
gayety  in  it  (not  even  in  diving  at  faces  or  pulling  beards),  seeming 
to  regard  it  as  solely  for  the  serious  purpose  of  putting  things  into 
her  mouth ;  she  would  sit  very  quietly,  looking  about  her  and 
putting  her  rattle,  ring,  or  spools  into  her  mouth.  Yet  twice 
(129th  day)  she  looked  at  her  mother  in  a  troubled  way  and  put 
up  her  lip  to  cry  on  losing  a  heavy  napkin  ring  she  was  trying  to 
lift,  —  an  unprecedented  sign  of  disappointment. 

She  grasped  with  the  mouth  with  more  precision  and  prompt- 
ness than  with  the  hands,  and  really  showed  more  disposition  to 
use  it.  The  123d  day,  and  again  the  128th,  as  I  held  her  in  my 
arms,  she  turned  around,  and  made  two  or  three  quick  dives  at 
me  with  her  head,  apparently  to  get  the  loose  folds  of  my  bodice 
into  her  mouth.  When  held  near  any  one's  face  she  would  attack 
it,  sometimes  with  a  sudden  dive  of  her  head  (accompanied  with  a 
sympathetic  doubling-up  movement  of  the  body),  and  mouth  it 
with  satisfaction.  She  still  dived  her  head  to  meet  her  fingers 
when  at  times  she  seemed  suddenly  to  think  of  putting  them  into 
her  mouth  (at  such  times  the  fingers  likewise  were  brought  up 
with  a  jerk,  though  all  other  voluntary  movements  of  the  hands 
were  slow).  The  130th  day,  as  she  lay  on  her  back,  a  rubber  ring 
in  her  mouth  fell  out  and  lay  around  her  nose,  resting  on  her 
cheeks,  upper  lip,  and  the  bridge  of  the  nose.  She  made  many 
efforts  to  seize  it  with  her  lips,  stretching  her  mouth  open  ridicu- 
lously, but  none  with  her  hands,  though  they  fluttered  wildly  in 
sympathy.  When  after  some  time  she  had  tumbled  it  off  with  a 
movement    of  her    head,   she   looked    up    at    her    mother    a    little 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  3  l  7 

frightened,  we  thought;  but  when  the  same  thing  happened  twice 
again  she  became  familiarized  with  it,  and  after  a  little  effort  to  get 
hold  of  the  ring  with  her  mouth,  let  it  lie  there. 

I  noticed  this  week  also  that  she  began  to  cling  a  little  to  us 
when  held  in  arms,  instead  of  sitting  passively  in  our  arms  as  in  a 
chair. 

At  the  end  of  the  nineteenth  week  and  in  the  twentieth  there 
was  a  wonderful  advance  in  skill  and  interest  in  grasping.  First 
(133d  day)  I  noticed  increased  promptness  and  precision  in  taking 
hold  of  small,  familiar  objects  in  familiar  positions,  and  also  increased 
skill  in  playing  with  a  short  rattle  set  on  a  rubber  strip,  by  taking 
the  rubber  between  her  jaws  and  jerking  it  out  with  a  twitch. 
Next  (133d  day),  I  noticed  that  she  could  always  find  a  toy  when 
she  had  dropped  it  on  the  tray  of  her  chair,  but  not  if  she  dropped 
it  in  her  lap;  in  that  case  she  would  make  a  little  wishful  cry  that 
always  brought  some  one  to  restore  the  toy,  —  again  to  the  dis- 
comfiture of  my  observations,  for  I  wished  to  see  if  she  would  not 
learn  to  find  it  herself.  The  same  day  in  making  one  of  her 
sudden  passes  with  open  mouth  at  my  forehead,  she  made  a 
simultaneous  clutch  at  my  face  with  her  hands,  and  this  was  the 
rule  for  two  or  three  days;  then  the  clutch  was  made  with  hands 
alone,  though  the  baby  would  afterward  put  up  her  lips  to  mouth 
our  faces.  The  134th  day  she  held  her  rattle  up  before  her  as  she 
lay  on  her  back,  and  inspected  it  carefully.  The  135th  day  she 
played  with  a  call-bell  with  great  interest,  pulling  it  toward  her 
and  trying  for  a  long  time  to  lift  it,  with  but  partial  success;  the 
same  day  she  held  an  envelope  a  long  time,  pulling  it  this  way  and 
that  between  her  two  hands  without  trying  to  put  it  into  her 
mouth;  the  same  day,  too,  she  first  reached  for  her  toes,  on  having 
her  attention  drawn  to  them.  She  had  been  shown  them  once 
before,  and  had  regarded  them  with  interest  but  had  made  no  effort 
to  touch  them;  now  she  took  hold  of  them  without  difficulty, 
and  —  which  surprised  me  — ■  flexed  the  foot  forward  on  the  ankle 
joint  and  brought  it  within  reach  of  her  hand  in  quite  a  skilful 
way,  as  though  she  felt  her  control  of  it;  she  did  this  several 
times.  Later  she  reached  for  her  toes  more  than  once  without 
suggestion,  and  also  took   hold   of  her  leg   repeatedly,  as  it  was 


3  I  ^>  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

kicked  up  before  her.  The  136th  day  she  played  assiduously  with 
her  feet  and  toes;  and  set  down  on  the  table,  she  tried  quite  hard 
to  scramble  forward  to  get  at  an  object  she  desired,  and  reached 
for  everything  near  her  on  the  table.  The  137th  day  she  was 
deeply  interested  in  playing  with  a  call-bell,  pulling  it  about, 
banging  it  down,  etc.  For  two  or  three  days  now,  though  the 
hand  was  still  put  out  for  grasping  slowly  and  cautiously,  it  was 
with  the  fingers  held  ready. 

By  the  end  of  the  week  (twentieth)  the  desire  to  seize  every- 
thing had  become  the  baby's  predominant  interest.  Moreover,  as 
may  be  seen  from  the  items  of  progress  day  by  day  given  just 
above,  it  was  no  longer  merely  for  the  sake  of  putting  things  to  her 
mouth  that  she  seized  them:  finger-touch,  together  with  the  varied 
muscular  experience  possible  through  manipulation,  seemed  to  be 
at  last  asserting  itself  as  a  source  of  interest  against  the  sensibility 
of  lips  and  tongue.  The  baby  would  "play"  with  many  things, 
pulling  them  to  and  fro  for  a  minute,  then  putting  to  her  mouth. 
Whatever  was  shown  that  interested  her  she  tried  to  seize,  — ■  pic- 
tures on  the  wall,  figures  on  a  tray,  roses  on  the  bedquilt.  If  an 
article  did  not  move  readily  under  her  hands  her  mouth  went  down 
to  it.  Though  very  fretful  for  a  couple  of  days,  from  a  temporary 
insufficiency  of  nourishment,  she  would  always  stop  fretting  for 
some  minutes  when  given  a  plaything. 

The  rest  of  the  fifth  month  is  a  record  of  incessant  interest  in 
seizing  and  handling  things,  and  of  some  increase  in  skill.  The 
baby  was  absorbed  in  looking  and  feeling;  she  desired  to  look  at 
and  touch  every  object  shown  her,  and  others  on  which  her  eyes 
might  light.  She  would  stretch  both  arms  out,  taking  hold  on  the 
two  sides,  and  usually  drew  the  object  at  once  toward  her  mouth  ; 
though  often  she  would  pull  it  around  awhile  first.  Her  toes  were 
always  seized  with  great  interest  when  she  was  undressed.  A  steel 
bell  given  her  on  the  146th  day  was  at  first  pushed  and  pulled 
about  on  the  table,  and  put  into  her  mouth,  —  any  part  that  came 
handiest,  —  but  by  the  149th  day  was  rung  with  intention;  she 
would  shake  it  awhile,  then  put  it  into  her  mouth,  then  shake  it 
again.  The  first  time  she  was  given  food  in  a  bottle  (141st  day) 
she  took  hold  of  the  bottle  readily  and  put  the  nipple  easily  into 


Shinn.]  77/i?  Development  of  a   Child.  319 

her  mouth;  then  (not  liking  the  taste  of  the  new  rubber  nipple) 
would  reject  it,  look  at  it  with  interest  and  surprise,  reach  out  her 
arms  for  it,  pull  it  to  her  mouth  and  get  the  nipple  in  nicely,  then 
go  through  the  same  process  again.  She  wished  to  hold  and 
manage  the  bottle  entirely  herself,  but  could  not  be  trusted  to  tilt 
it  properly.  A  large  glass  marble,  perhaps  an  inch  and  a  half  or 
more  in  diameter,  which  she  had  failed  to  hold  on  the  140th  day, 
she  lifted  nicely  with  both  hands  on  the  141st  and  carried  success- 
fully to  her  mouth,  seeming  to  appreciate  perfectly  that  it  was  a 
slippery  and  difficult  handful;  she  lost  it  easily,  however,  and  did 
not  care  much  for  it.  Later  she  picked  it  up  with  one  hand,  but 
could  not  keep  it. 

I  noticed  (147th  day)  that  in  taking  up  her  bell  she  did  not 
nearly  always  reverse  her  thumb  properly,  as  when  a  small,  con- 
venient object  was  placed  in  her  hand;  nor  did  she  always  use  her 
whole  hand,  but  would  get  hold  with  two  fingers  or  more,  as  it 
might  chance. 

By  the  last  week  of  the  month  she  played  with  as  small  an 
object  as  the  curtain  cord,  especially  the  knot  on  the  end  of  it. 

I  never  at  any  time  saw  her  make  a  snatch  at  an  object  within 
her  reach  and  miss  it,  like  Preyer's  boy,  by  reaching  too  far  to  left 
or  right,  or  too  short.  She  sometimes  reached  for  something  a 
little  too  far  away,  but  never  from  the  first  more  than  three  feet 
away,  and  her  accuracy  in  estimating  her  reach  rapidly  increased. 
Her  method  of  seizing  by  a  cautious  extension  of  both  hands, 
corralling  the  object,  so  to  speak,  between  them,  made  it  impossible 
to  make  many  errors. 

During  the  fifth  month,  the  gesture  of  holding  out  her  hands  to 
be  taken  was  developed  from  grasping ;  and  also  another  interesting 
gesture,  that  of  holding  out  the  arms  to  an  object  rather  as  an 
expression  of  interest  and  desire  than  as  a  real  effort  to  grasp  it. 
In  the  case  of  these  gestures,  (both  of  which  were  first  noticed  in 
the  nineteenth  week),  as  they  differentiated  more  and  more  from 
grasping,  the  arms  were  more  and  more  reached  toward  objects  at 
a  considerable  distance. 

Through  the  sixth  month  the  baby  was  eager  to  get  at  and 
touch  everything;  when  carried  about  the  room  she  put  out  her 


320  University  of   California.  [Vol.  i- 

hands  continually,  wishing  to  touch,  pull,  and  carry  to  her  mouth 
every  object;  she  was  no  longer  satisfied  simply  to  watch.  As 
she  sat  in  our  laps  she  was  busy  reaching  in  every  direction  for 
something  to  get  hold  of.  If  nothing  presented  itself  within 
reach  to  seize,  she  would  lean  this  way  and  that  as  if  seeking 
something.  Sitting  on  the  floor  she  would  at  this  time  look 
around  her  for  dropped  playthings,  and  could  pick  them  up  if  they 
lay  conveniently.  In  carrying  objects  to  her  mouth  she  made  no 
more  errors:  from  the  153d  day  I  noticed  that  she  did  it  promptly 
and  correctly.  The  same  day  a  bright  celluloid  ball,  perhaps  two 
inches  in  diameter,  was  held  out  to  her:  she  reached  out  both 
hands  and  took  it  between  them  and  carried  it  to  her  lips,  and  this 
was  several  times  repeated.  I  then  held  up  to  the  light  the  large 
and  heavy  glass  marble  which  she  had  managed  successfully  with 
both  hands  on  the  141st  day.  Holding  the  celluloid  ball  in  one 
hand,  she  reached  for  the  marble,  got  it  firmly  in  the  other  hand, 
and  brought  both  at  once  to  her  mouth.  This  also  was  repeated 
several  times.  She  had  evidently  quite  got  over  any  difficulty  in 
managing  different  objects  in  her  two  hands.  On  the  158th  day 
she  tried  to  catch  flies  on  the  window,  reaching  her  arms  high  after 
them  as  they  crept  up  the  pane.  The  163d  day  she  handled  and  bit 
a  small  rubber  ring  (such  as  is  put  about  papers,  —  perhaps  ^  of 
an  inch  in  diameter,  the  rubber  about  rV  of  an  inch  in  thickness), 
and  in  the  twenty-fourth  week  was  devoted  to  strings  as  playthings. 
One  particular  case  of  skill,  which  was  remarkable  as  involving 
the  use  of  a  number  of  other  muscles  in  co-operation  with  those 
of  hands,  arms,  and  neck,  was  the  trick  of  putting  the  toe  in  the 
mouth,  which  began  in  the  fifth  month  and  reached  great  perfection 
in  the  sixth.  The  baby  tried  it  first  on  the  148th  day,  and  made 
great  efforts,  pulling  the  toes  toward  her  mouth,  but  would  always 
kick  and  lose  them  out.  Then  she  took  the  toes  of  the  left  foot 
with  the  left  hand,  and  clasped  the  right  hand  about  the  instep  to 
hold  the  foot  still,  and  so  pulled  it  quite  near  to  her  mouth,  but 
always  lost  it  at  the  last  moment.  Finally  she  clasped  the  ankle 
and  heel  firmly  with  both  hands,  and  so,  after  several  attempts, 
brought  the  toe  triumphantly  into  her  mouth,  —  losing  it  instantly, 
apparently  by  diversion  of  attention,  as  she  looked  up  to  us  for 
sympathy  in  her  success.     The  performance  was  repeated  at  once, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  321 

however.  After  this,  she  managed  it  daily  better,  and  was  more 
interested  in  her  toes:  I  was  told  that  on  the  155th  day  she  caught 
sight  of  her  feet  when  in  her  high-chair,  and  leaned  over  its  tray 
and  grasped  them.  By  the  159th  she  got  the  toe  into  her  mouth 
very  skilfully,  holding  the  foot  with  both  hands  firmly  and  getting 
the  big  toe  between  her  jaws,  where  she  would  suck  it  assiduously 
for  as  long  as  her  mother  would  let  her  (which  was  never  over  a 
minute).  She  would  hold  the  toe  firmly  to  her  mouth  with  both 
hands  all  the  time,  and  even  when  it  was  pulled  or  shaken  out, 
would  hold  it  "handy  by."  Her  incessant  occupation  when 
undressed  was  now  this  sucking  of  her  toe,  varied  with  a  few  kicks 
and  flourishes  of  her  legs.  The  167th  day,  the  instant  her  skirts 
were  drawn  off,  her  legs  went  up  as  if  a  spring  were  released,  and 
her  hands  went  to  her  toes.  At  first  her  inability  to  keep  the  foot 
from  kicking  away  from  her  as  if  it  were  a  foreign  object,  unless 
forcibly  held  by  her  hands,  showed  very  limited  co-operation  of  the 
different  parts  of  her  body:  but  by  this  time  (note  of  168th  day) 
she  used  lags  and  hands,  head  and  mouth,  very  well  together. 
After  the  167th  day,  the  interest  in  sucking  the  toe  declined, 
though  the  baby  continued  for  a  couple  of  weeks  or  more  to  seize 
at  her  feet,  which  flew  up  the  instant  they  were  released  in 
undressing;  yet  the  movement  was  made  as  if  by  a  mechanical 
habit,  without  much  real  attention  to  the  feet. 

I  have  said  that  even  before  the  close  of  the  fifth  month  objects 
were  not  grasped  merely  to  be  carried  to  the  mouth,  but  "played 
with"  a  little,  by  moving  them  about  in  simple  ways  :  this  increased 
through  the  sixth  and  seventh  months,  and  at  seven  months  I  saw 
the  baby  once  or  twice  playing  with  something  without  trying  to 
put  it  into  her  mouth  at  all.  This  was  rare,  however,  even  to  the  end 
of  the  eighth  month:  it  was  but  slowly  that  the  hand  superseded 
the  mouth  as  the  chief  seat  of  active  touch,  —  far  more  slowly  than 
it  had  taken  the  lead  for  purposes  of  prehension ;  and  far  on  into 
the  second  year  there  were  recurrences  of  the  disposition  to  carry 
everything  to  the  mouth.     (See  Sense  of  Contact,  pp.  137,  138.) 

In  the  seventh  month  there  was  perhaps  not  quite  such  an 
ardent  absorption  in  seizing  at  anything  and  everything  as  in  the 


322  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

sixth,  but  I  have  incessant  notes  of  the  baby's  reaching  for  objects 
that  interested  her.  On  the  202c!  day,  I  was  told  (but  did  not  see 
it  myself),  she  caught  sight  of  a  flower  out  of  the  corner  of  her  eye, 
and  readied  backward  and  snatched  it,  without  turning  to  bring  it 
to  the  field  of  direct  vision.  It  is  not  until  the  beginning  of  the 
eighth  month  that  I  find  any  note  of  the  swift  snatching  by  which 
babies  elude  the  vigilance  of  their  guardians,  and  get  hold  of  things 
before  they  can  be  stopped :  at  this  time  she  would  thus  snatch 
"like  a  shot"  at  the  dog's  hair  if  he  came  near,  and  pull  it,  —  a 
considerably  easier  thing,  of  course,  than  to  snatch  thus  swiftly, 
with  correct  aim,  at  a  single  small  object. 

By  the  end  of  the  seventh  month  the  baby's  advance  in  locomo- 
tion was  displacing  her  interest  in  grasping.  The  power  was  fairly 
acquired  for  life,  and  now  took  its  normal  place  as  one  among 
her  resources.  It  continued,  however,  throughout  the  first  and 
second  years,  and  to  some  extent  even  in  the  third,  to  be  more  used 
than  it  is  by  grown  people:  it  was  more  necessary  for  the  child 
than  for  us  to  get  hold  of 'an  object  instead  of  being  satisfied  with 
looking  at  it. 

Her  skill  in  grasping  when  the  finger-tips  alone  could  be  used, 
perfected  but  slowly:  in  the  thirty-fourth  week  she  tried  unsuccess- 
fully to  pick  up  tiny  scraps  and  thrums,  say  ]^  inch  in  diameter, 
from  the  floor;  in  the  thirty-sixth  (249th  day),  she  picked  up  a 
small  tack  and  a  pin  with  ease;  and  at  thirty-eight  weeks  old  she 
played  with  a  single  hair  and  with  a  wisp  of  thread  scarcely  larger 
than  a  pinhead.  Even  at  fifteen  months  old,  however,  I  saw  her 
make  several  efforts  before  she  could  pick  up  a  small  shot. 

Differentiation    of    the    Forefinger    Tip   as    Special    Organ    of 

Active    Feeling. 

I  saw  no  sign  of  special  delicacy  of  feeling  in  the  finger  tip  till 
grasping  had  been  thoroughly  established  for  some  months.  On 
the  278th  day  (tenth  month)  I  first  saw  the  baby  investigating  some 
object  with  her  forefinger  tip;  again  the  next  day ;  and  on  the  next 
the  forefinger  was  much  used,  or  sometimes  the  thumb  and  forefinger 
together.  The  next  day  the  use  of  the  right  forefinger  was  very 
marked:  it  was  often  carried  extended  even  when  the  baby  was  not 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  323 

investigating,  and  when  the  other  ringers  were  half  closed;  the  left 
forefinger,  however,  was  not  kept  separate  from  the  others.  Out  of 
this  special  use  of  the  right  forefinger,  pointing  was  developed,  as  I 
shall  elsewhere  describe. 

Use  of  Right  and  Left  Hand. 

In  the  involuntary  movements  that  preceded  grasping  I  was 
not  able  to  detect  any  predominance  of  either  side  of  the  body. 
Either  hand  closed  equally  well  on  an  object  laid  in  the  palm,  and 
in  the  various  spontaneous  and  expressive  movements  of  the  early 
months  there  seemed  no  difference  in  activity  between  right  and 
left  limbs.  The  disposition  to  use  both  hands  together  was 
noticeable  in  all  stages  of  progress  toward  grasping  and  for  a  time 
after  grasping  was  acquired,  —  up  to  the  middle  of  the  fifth  month 
at  least.  In  fact,  the  early  grasping  may  be  in  general  described 
as  two-handed,  like  a  squirrel's;  even  when  one  hand  did  the 
actual  grasping,  it  was  after  both  hands  had  fumbled  for  the  object, 
and  whichever  touched  it  first  had  the  preference.  When  in  the 
third  month  the  baby  was  pulling  herself  up  to  a  sitting  position 
by  holding  to  our  forefingers,  I  never  observed  any  difference 
between  the  hands  in  strength  of  pull.  When  in  the  fourth  month, 
upon  the  hands  encountering  each  other,  each  would  seize  and  try 
to  carry  the  other  to  the  mouth,  I  never  saw  that  either  one 
exercised  any  predominance. 

In  several  incidents  where  one  hand  or  the  other  seemed  to  be 
preferred,  it  always  proved  to  be  due  to  mere  accident,  or  to  be 
contradicted  by  some  other  incident.  The  relative  use  of  right  and 
left  hand  in  managing  the  foot,  e.  g.,  (p.  320)  was  entirely  due  to 
the  position  in  which  the  mother  always  held  the  baby  while  un- 
dressing her,  leaving  the  left  leg  freer,  and  exposed  to  fuller  light 
than  the  right ;  when  the  position  was  reversed  for  experiment,  the 
other  leg  was  seized.  In  the  sixth  month,  when  the  baby  began  to 
roll  over  (p.  333),  it  was  always  leftward,  even  when  her  toys  were 
placed  on  the  right,  —  indicating  possibly  that  the  muscles  on  the 
right  were  stronger;  yet  it  may  have  simply  chanced  that  the  first 
time  she  rolled  over  was  in  this  direction,  and  she  had  no  idea  of 
accomplishing  the  movement  in  any  other  way.  After  her  mother 
had  once  helped  her  to  roll  over  toward  the  right,  she  rolled  one 
22 


324  University  of  California.  [Vol  i. 

way  or  the  other  indifferently.  In  the  eighth  month  (228th  day) 
she  held  a  valise  ten  seconds  with  her  left  hand,  and  six  seconds 
only  and  with  more  appearance  of  effort,  with  the  right,  —  but  the 
left  hand  was  used  first,  and  there  was  probably  some  general 
fatigue  before  the  weight  was  taken  with  the  right  hand. 

Nevertheless,  we  were  satisfied  that  in  the  first  weeks  of  grasping 
the  left  hand  was  more  used ;  but  I  cannot  establish  this  from  my 
record.  There  is  no  question  that  after  the  two-handed  period  was 
fairly  over,  after  grasping  had  become  habitual,  the  right  hand  was 
the  one  used  by  preference,  and  has  been  consistently  so  ever  since. 
When  the  use  of  the  forefinger  tip  for  special  investigation  appeared, 
in  the  tenth  month,  it  was  always  the  right  forefinger;  and  so  too 
in  pointing  afterward.  It  is  my  impression  that  it  was  at  this  time 
that  distinct  right-handedness  became  apparent;  but  my  notes  do 
not  make  this  clear. 

In  climbing  upstairs  in  the  twenty-first  month  the  right  foot 
always  led,  —  probably  in  all  the  climbing,  but  I  failed  to  observe 
as  to  this.  In  first  using  a  fork  at  table  (fifteenth  month)  and  later 
in  all  implement-using,  and  in  giving  her  hand,  the  child  always 
used  the  right  hand  as  a  matter  of  course,  without  instruction.  In 
many  cases  where  I  have  not  recorded  which  hand  was  used,  or 
which  foot  led,  I  am  satisfied  I  should  have  noted  it  had  it  been  the 
left,  since  the  use  of  the  right  was  after  the  age  of  eight  or  nine 
months  so  common  and  apparently  so  instinctive  with  her;  and 
since  I  myself  am  strongly  right-handed.  The  child's  parents  are 
both  right-handed,  the  mother  strongly  so;  the  father's  mother  and 
brother  have  a  decided  ambidexterity,  which  seems  to  remain  from 
an  original  left-handedness,  out  of  which  they  have  been  trained; 
and  an  uncle  of  the  mother's  is  completely  left-handed. 


Equilibrium  and  Locomotion. 

The  main  movements  by  which  the  powers  of  balancing  and 
carrying  the  body  in  human  fashion  were  acquired  (holding  up 
and  turning  the  head,  sitting,  rolling,  creeping,  standing,  walking, 
running,  climbing,  jumping)  I  have  not  separated  into  topical 
chapters,  since  a  better  idea  of  the  progressive  development  of 
command  over  the  body  can  be  had  from  a  continuous  narrative. 
The  movements  unfolded  to  some  extent  one  from  another,  in  a 
continuous  progress. 

First  Two  Months:    Holding  up   and   Turning  the    Head,  and 
Progress  toward  Sitting. 

From  quite  early,  perhaps  even  from  the  first  week,  (I  began  to 
notice  it  in  the  second)  there  was  perceptible  in  the  bath  a  tenser 
or  more  innervated  condition  of  the  muscles,  and  especially  of  the 
neck  muscles;  by  the  third  week  there  was  an  appearance  of 
voluntary  effort  in  this  stiffening  of  the  neck,  and  when  the  baby's 
body  was  held  horizontally  in  the  water,  her  head  supported  just 
over  the  surface  by  her  mother's  fingers,  she  would  raise  it  slightly 
from  the  support.  The  ability  thus  to  raise  the  head  increased 
steadily  without  any  marked  step  of  progress.  Meanwhile  the 
vague  spontaneous  rolling  of  the  head  to  and  fro  took  on  more 
appearance  of  real  turning  in  order  to  stare  about;  in  the  fourth 
week  (25th  day)  the  head  was  unmistakably  thrown  backward  in 
order  to  see  something  better  (see  Fixation,  p.   14). 

In  the  fifth  week  blind  seeking  movements  with  the  head  were 
made  when  the  baby  was  placed  at  the  breast ;  there  was  a  trick  of 
turning  the  head  sidewise  in  looking  at  the  mother;  once  (33d  day) 
when  the  baby  was  laid  on  her  face,  she  turned  her  head  sidewise 
to  free  the  face,  at  the  same  time  propping  her  body  up  somewhat 
with  her  knees;  on  the  33d  day  the  head  was  unmistakably  turned 
to  follow  the  movement  of  a  candle. 

By  the  end  of  the  fifth  week  the  head  was  held  up  from  the 
mother's  hand  in  the  bath  for  twenty  seconds  at  a  time,  at  an  angle 
of  about  forty-five  degrees  with  the  line  of  the  back.     At  the  same 

(325) 


326  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

time  the  body  was  strongly  propped  with  the  knees  on  the  bottom 
of  the  tub.  It  was  no  longer  in  the  bath  only  that  the  head  was 
raised:  the  baby  would  lift  it  to  see  around,  sometimes  for  twenty 
seconds  at  a  time,  when  she  was  held  upright  against  the  shoulder. 
By  the  end  of  the  sixth  week,  when  held  thus  against  the  shoulder, 
or  upright  in  the  arms,  she  would  hold  her  head  erect,  balancing 
it  perfectly  for  perhaps  a  quarter  minute,  and  even  then  would 
not  drop  it  limply,  but  seek  some  neighboring  support  to  rest  it 
against. 

In  the  seventh  week  she  could  hold  her  head  up  in  the  bath 
fairly  at  right  angles  with  the  line  of  her  back,  for  a  quarter  minute 
or  more.  She  would  rest  it  sidewise  on  her  mother's  wrist  when 
tired,  then  raise  it  again.  While  holding  it  up,  she  would  keep  it 
turning  from  side  to  side  all  the  time,  —  a  movement  which  I  have 
classed  as  spontaneous,  (p.  300)  but  which  I  mention  again  here, 
since  the  power  to  balance  and  turn  the  head  at  the  same  time  was 
now  first  seen. 

One  day  in  this  week  (45th  day)  the  baby  turned  her  head  to 
look  at  the  piano  keys,  —  the  first  unmistakable  use  of  the  move- 
ment to  bring  something  within  the  field  of  vision. 

Meanwhile  a  pleasure  in  having  the  body  also  erect  had  become 
apparent.  The  baby's  liking  for  upright  positions  ■ —  for  a  sitting 
one  in  which  her  grandmother  held  her,  and  for  the  position  up 
against  some  one's  shoulder  —  had  been  noted  as  early  as  the  sixth 
week,  and  in  the  seventh  (47th  day)  I  noticed  that  when  fretting 
persistently  she  stopped  when  I  lifted  her  to  my  shoulder.  This 
liking  now  began  to  develop  into  desire:  in  the  ninth  week  the 
baby  would  usually  begin  to  fret  after  lying  down  some  twenty 
minutes,  and  would  seem  satisfied  when  lifted  and  held  against  the 
shoulder.  On  the  57th  day,  when  thus  held,  she  would  first  raise 
her  head  erect,  then  straighten  back  against  the  arm  that  supported 
her  till  her  back  also  was  erect,  (propped  by  the  arm  or  hand 
behind  to  be  sure,  but  still  taking  much  of  its  own  weight,)  and 
would  then  occupy  herself  in  looking  about.  This  she  did  persist- 
ently every  time  she  was  taken  up.  As  often  as  pressed  down  on 
the  shoulder  she  would  become  discontented  and  straighten  herself 
up  again.     She  would  hold  the  head  erect  for  a  half  minute,  then 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  2>27 

lean  it  against  my  cheek  for  a  few  seconds,  raise  it  again,  drop  it 
forward  on  her  breast,  hold  it  up,  drop  it  backward, —  not  helplessly, 
but  as  if  to  look  at  the  ceiling,  —  and  again  raise  it,1  always  in  high 
content  till  made  to  lie  down. 

Here  was  a  distinct  step  toward  sitting,  in  the  desire  and  ability 
to  hold  the  spine  as  well  as  the  head  erect;  and  it  continued  daily, 
—  the  power  to  balance  the  head  slowly  increasing  at  the  same 
time.  On  the  59th  day  the  head  was  held  up  (when  I  timed  it 
once)  two  and  a  quarter  minutes;  it  wavered  a  little  during  this 
time,  sinking  somewhat  forward  or  to  one  side,  but  most  of  the 
time  was  quite  erect.  The  60th  day  it  was  held  up  with  some 
wavering,  but  without  once  sinking  to  any  support,  for  many 
minutes.  By  the  second  week  of  the  third  month  the  head  was 
balanced  quite  perfectly,  and  turned  at  will  to  look  about.2 

Third  Month  to  End  of  the  Half  Year:  Sitting,  Turning  Over, 
and  Primitive  Leg  and  Trunk  Movements. 

The  desire  for  the  sitting  position  now,  with  the  beginning  of 
the  third  month,  became  most  persistent  and  striking.  The  baby 
for  a  time  made  no  effort  to  raise  herself,  but  would  fret  and  some- 
times cry  hard  till  she  was  raised  and  held  in  a  sitting  position. 
Her  back  was  always  supported  carefully,  but  once  when  an 
inexperienced  person  neglected  this  precaution  for  a  minute,  steady- 
ing her  only,  she  held  herself  up  firmly,  and  seemed  to  like  it.  By 
the  twelfth  week  her  insistence  had  led  to  her  spending  much  of 

1  It  was  at  this  time  that  the  curious  jerking  and  turning  movements  refi  rred 
to  above  (p.  301)  occurred.  On  the  56th  day,  while  thus  holding  her  head  erect, 
the  baby  began  to  jerk  it  to  left  and  right  as  far  as  the  neck  would  turn,  so 
strongly  that  when  I  did  not  keep  my  face  out  of  the  way  I  received  a  really 
smart  blow.  She  would  let  her  head  drop  on  my  shoulder  for  about  two 
seconds,  then  raise  it  and  jerk  it  about  again;  this  she  kept  up  for  five  or  ten 
minutes.  The  59th  day,  while  holding  up  her  head  with  persistent  effort,  she 
turned  it  continuously  from  side  to  side  for  about  a  minute,  this  time  without  the 
jerking  motion,  and  with  some  appearance  of  deliberate  experiment, — quite  a 
different  movement  in  manner  from  the  semi-spontaneous  turning  to  and  fro 
in  the  bath,  above  referred  to  (p.  326). 

-Of  the  vigorous  and  skilful  movements  of  the  neck  in  the  fourth,  fifth  and 
sixth  months,  in  connection  with  grasping,  I  have  given  a  record  above. 


328  University  of  California.  [Vol.  t. 

her  time  propped  up  with  cushions,  and  in  our  laps  she  was  always 
held  sitting. 

As  far  back  as  the  seventh  week,  when  the  mother  held  her 
hands  in  wiping  her  after  the  bath,  and  perhaps  gave  a  slight 
unintentional  pull,  the  baby  had  begun  to  pull,  as  if  to  lift  herself, 
though  she  probably  had  no  such  intent  at  that  time  (p.  312): 
her  mother  had  tried  to  develop  this,  however,  giving  the  baby  her 
fingers  and  starting  her  by  a  suggestive  little  tug;  and  she  had 
doubtless  had  the  experience  more  than  once  of  finding  herself 
raised  a  little  in  this  way.  She  now,  in  the  twelfth  week,  certainly 
made  efforts  to  raise  herself,  pulling  with  energy  on  her  mother's 
forefingers  when  one  was  laid  in  each  hand.  She  tried  also 
to  lift  her  body  directly  by  the  abdominal  muscles,  raising  her 
head  and  shoulders  from  the  lap  or  lounge  but  getting  no 
further   up. 

A  few  days  before  the  close  of  the  month  (89th  day)  her  parents 
called  me,  saying  that  the  baby  had  raised  herself  to  a  sitting 
position.  She  was  lying  slightly  reclined  along  her  mother's 
knees,  between  which  her  hips  were  somewhat  braced,  giving  her 
an  advantage  in  lifting  herself.  While  I  looked,  she  raised  her 
head  and  shoulders,  then  her  body,  with  much  appearance  of  effort, 
till  she  was  sitting.  She  did  this  twice,  and  tried  a  third  time,  but 
was  evidently  tired,  and  failed.  She  did  it  calmly  and  seriously, 
without  any  sign  of  pleasure  or  pride  in  the  exertion,  evidently 
for  the  mere  purpose  of  getting  into  a  sitting  position.  The  next 
day  her  mother  laid  her  along  her  lap  again  to  see  if  she  would 
repeat  the  action,  but  the  hips  were  not  so  well  braced,  and  though 
the  baby  tried  to  lift  herself  she  did  not  succeed.  Later,  as  she 
lay  across  her  mother's  lap,  she  kept  trying  to  lift  herself,  raising 
head  and  shoulders,  till  I  took  her  hands  to  give  her  a  leverage:  she 
tugged  at  my  hands  with  such  zeal  that  her  face  grew  red,  and 
raised  her  head  and  back,  but  the  legs  also  rose,  overweighted  by 
the  trunk,  and  she  could  not  raise  the  hips  and  buttocks;  she  fell 
back,  but  immediately  tried  again  very  earnestly,  and  still  a  third 
time;  then  she  became  discouraged,  and  began  to  fret  pitifully,  on 
which  her  mother  interfered  and  lifted  her  up.  Once  again  before 
night,  as  she  lay  reclined  a  little  on  a  pillow,  she  raised  herself 
several  times  till  her  shoulders  and  her  back  as  far  as  the  lumbar 
recrion  were  clear. 


Shinn.j  The  Development  of  a    Child.  329 

After  this  followed  several  days  of  persistent  efforts  to  raise 
herself.  On  the  last  day  of  the  third  month  I  found  the  baby 
lying  in  a  somewhat  reclining  position  and  trying  from  time  to 
time  to  sit  up:  I  gave  her  my  forefingers  to  pull  on,  and  finally, 
with  great  effort  and  eagerness,  she  pulled  herself  fairly  up,  and 
immediately  tipped  over  sidewise.  Afterward  she  tried  twenty- 
five  times,  with  scarcely  a  pause  between,  to  lift  herself  by  the 
abdominal  muscles,  but  could  lift  her  head  and  shoulders  only; 
she  showed  discouragement  by  this  time,  but  might  have  gone  on 
trying  had  she  been  allowed.  I  was  told  that  earlier  in  the  day, 
when  somewhat  higher  on  her  pillow,  she  had  thus,  by  the  sheer 
pull  of  the  abdominal  muscles,  brought  herself  up  sitting,  and  at 
once  tipped  forward  on  her  nose.  The  effort  and  earnestness  of 
all  these  attempts  to  sit  up  was  astonishing ;  and  when  the  baby 
pulled  on  my  hands  the  strength  of  her  grasp  and  pull  was  some- 
thing most  unexpected,  —  fully  equal  in  proportion  to  her  weight 
to  that  of  any  grown  person. 

With  the  fourth  month  the  progress  in  sitting  seemed  checked. 
The  baby  had  possibly  become  tired  or  discouraged  by  her  futile 
efforts  to  raise  herself.  During  the  month  she  would  occasionally 
try  to  lift  herself,  either  by  the  abdominal  muscles  unaided,  or  by 
pulling  on  our  fingers ;  and  by  the  latter  method  could  raise  her- 
self half-way,  but  never  quite  erect.  At  just  sixteen  weeks  old, 
when  propped  in  a  lounge  corner  almost  erect,  she  raised  herself 
completely,  and  sat  for  ten  minutes  or  more  with  apparent  ease 
and  perfect  balance;  but  I  found  that  the  cushions  and  lounge 
back  made  a  support  for  her  hips.  Twice,  however,  (once  in  the 
fifteenth  week  and  once  in  the  seventeenth,)  she  sat  for  a  few 
seconds  in  some   one's  lap  quite  unsupported  and   unsteadied. 

During  the  early  part  of  this  month,  her  determination  to  be 
always  sitting  or  else  held  upright  was  as  strong  as  in  the  third, 
and  nothing  but  having  her  legs  free  to  kick  could  reconcile  her 
to  being  laid  down  when  indoors  ;  but  by  the  middle  of  the  month, 
sitting  being  now  customary,  she  did  not  object  to  being  laid  down 
for  a  few  minutes  at  a  time ;  in  the  fifteenth  week,  when  laid  on 
the  floor  face  downward,  she  would  enjoy  the  novel  position  for 
a  short  time,  being  now  able  to  raise  her  head  and  shoulders 
and  look  around.     Up  to  the  seventeenth  week,  she  was  content 


3SO  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

to  lie  down  in  her  baby-carriage  outdoors ;  but  in  that  week  she 
rebelled,  and  fretted  till  allowed  to  sit  up.  The  first  time  that 
this  happened  she  shortly  became  very  sleepy,  but  though  her 
head  drooped  forlornly  she  would  not  be  laid  down,  protesting 
whenever  I  tried  it,  half-asleep  as  she  was. 

At  four  months  old,  in  deference  to  an  old  frontier  custom 
urged  by  the  grandfather,  the  baby  was  seated  daily,  with  her 
playthings,  in  a  horse-collar:  it  proved  a  comfortable  seat,  afford- 
ing just  the  support  for  her  hips  that  enabled  her  to  sit  erect,  and 
she  would  sit  very  quietly  for  twenty  minutes  at  a  time,  looking 
about  and  putting  her  rattle  or  spools  into  her  mouth.  Her 
experiments  in  the  nineteenth  and  twentieth  weeks  at  bending 
her  body  back  over  this  horse-collar,  and  afterward  over  the  arm 
of  any  one  who  held  her,  have  been  noted  above  (pp.  143,  198). 
By  this  time  she  required  but  the  slightest  steadying  to  sit  erect, 
and  often  sat  on  our  laps  for  a  few  minutes  without  real  support, 
though  steadied  by  a  hand.  In  the  nineteenth  week,  she  was 
delighted  by  being  seated  astride  one's  foot,  held  by  her  hands, 
and  swung  up  and  down  with  the  foot.  She  was  at  this  period 
coaxed  through  the  first  stages  of  drowsiness  by  being  seated  on 
the  point  of  the  knee  and  trotted,  as  noted  under  Sleep.  The 
strength  of  her  back  was  evidently  quite  adequate  to  sitting  alone, 
but  the  balancing  was  not  mastered. 

(In  this  week,  the  nineteenth,  began  the  progress  toward  loco- 
motion, in  the  first  rolling  and  sprawling  movements  on  the  floor: 
the  development  of  these  now  went  on  alongside  that  of  sitting, 
and  will  be  reverted  to  presently.) 

On  the  139th  day  she  sat  quite  alone  on  my  lap  for  some  ten 
seconds  ;  the  next  day  several  times  for  a  full  minute:  she  could  sit 
alone  very  well  when  perfectly  still,  but  if  she  turned  about,  reached 
for  anything,  or  even  flourished  her  hands  or  feet,  she  would  topple 
over.  When  seated  in  her  baby-carriage  two  days  later,  she  began 
to  play  with  the  leather  strap  that  passed  in  front  to  prevent  her 
falling  out,  and  sat  erect  for  nearly  half  an  hour  without  other 
support  than  the  contact  of  her  hands  (and  sometimes  her  mouth) 
with  this.  Sitting  quite  alone  for  a  minute  or  two  now  became 
common,  —  or  for  longer  periods  with  but  the  slightest  pretext 
of  support;  once,  on  the  143d  day,  losing  her  balance,  she  recov- 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  331 

ered  it  instead  of  tipping  over.  This  sitting  alone,  however,  was 
always  thus  far  on  the  lap  or  a  cushion,  or  some  other  hollowed 
or  yielding  surface:  she  could  not  keep  her  balance  on  the  floor 
or  table  for  more  than  a  few  seconds. 

The  sixth  month  showed  gradual  improvement  in  equilibrium  in 
sitting,  with  no  marked  step  of  advance.  At  twenty-two  weeks  old 
she  could  not  sit  alone  on  the  floor  more  than  half  a  minute,  —  she 
could  hold  herself  erect  without  difficulty,  but  could  not  keep 
equilibrium,  and  would  topple  as  soon  as  she  reached  for  anything, 
though  she  was  now  quite  secure  in  her  balance  for  several  minutes 
on  the  lap.  At  twenty-five  weeks,  she  sat  pretty  steadily  on  a  stool 
for  a  good  many  seconds,  not  even  toppling  over  when  she  sneezed. 
On  the  last  day  of  the  month  (twenty-sixth  week)  she  sat  for  nine 
minutes  on  the  lap,  and  six  minutes  on  the  floor,  and  in  neither  case 
showed  any  insecurity  at  the  end  of  the  time,  but  became  restless. 
When  not  kept  perpendicular  by  judiciously  dangled  playthings, 
however,  she  still  was  sure  to  tilt  herself  over  before  long  by  reach- 
ing after  something,  or  by  throwing  up  her  feet  to  catch  at  her  toes. 
On  the  lap  she  often  sat  for  long  times  without  any  help.  From 
high  reclining  positions  —  perhaps  half  sitting  —  she  could  sit  up 
with  ease;  but  during  the  whole  of  this  and  the  preceding  month 
(except  for  a  slight  renewal  at  twenty  weeks  old)  efforts  to  raise 
herself  when  lying  down  had  been  given  up. 

This  may  have  been  partly  because  lying  down  became  more 
agreeable  now  that  she  could  move  about  more  freely.  I  have 
mentioned  above  that  in  the  nineteenth  week  the  first  locomotor 
movements  had  appeared.  At  that  time  the  baby  had  begun  rather 
suddenly  to  turn  and  change  her  position  a  good  deal  when  lying 
on  the  floor,  —  not  to  roll  completely  over,  but  to  get  from  back 
to  side,  and  by  sundry  slight,  irregular  movements  to  get  into  quite 
varied  positions.  What  with  this  rudimentary  rolling  and  wriggling 
on  the  floor,  the  freer  turning,  leaning,  bending,  and  recovering  in 
sitting,  and  the  movements  connected  with  grasping,  especially  the 
complicated  ones  in  reaching  for  and  handling  her  feet,  it  is  evident 
that  by  the  latter  part  of  the  fifth  month,  the  trunk  and  limbs  were 
coming  into  a  condition  of  control  and  flexibility  favorable  to  the 
later  achievement  of  creeping,  standing,  and  walking.     The  desire 


3$2  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

of  locomotion  had  also  to  be  developed  considerably  before  the 
baby  would  put  herself  to  the  necessary  effort  of  acquiring  it ;  just 
as  a  great  desire  to  sit  up  was  necessary  before  the  effortless  position 
of  lying  down  could  be  abandoned  for  the  difficult  balancing  and 
muscular  exertions  of  sitting.  The  first  rudiment  of  desire  of 
locomotion  appeared  in  the  twentieth  week,  and  in  connection  with 
grasping :  on  the  1 36th  day,  as  the  baby  sat  on  the  table,  she  began 
to  lean  forward  on  my  arm,  which  encircled  her,  till  I  let  her  sink- 
down  on  her  hands  and  knees ;  and  in  this  position  she  tried  quite 
hard  to  strain  or  scramble  forward  to  reach  some  desired  object, 
pushing  backward  with  her  feet,  but  did  not  actually  move  forward. 
She  had  had  incidentally  a  good  deal  of  experience  of  being  moved 
forward  by  pushing  with  her  feet  (in  the  bath-tub,  c.  g.),  and  this 
movement  may  have  been  the  result  of  that  experience,  or  may 
have  been  merely  an  accompanying  movement,  in  the  effort  to 
strain  forward. 

During  the  first  half  of  the  sixth  month  the  sprawling  about  had 
increased  in  freedom,  and  was  enjoyed  for  fifteen  or  twenty  minutes 
at  a  time  by  the  baby;  but  showed  no  tendency  to  develop  into  any 
regular  method  of  locomotion.  From  the  fifteenth  week,  the 
grandmother  (believing  creeping  an  important  preparation  for  walk- 
ing, better  for  limbs  and  back  than  any  of  the  substitutes,  such  as 
hitching)  had  placed  the  baby  on  the  floor,  face  downward,  to  give 
her  an  opportunity  to  develop  creeping  movements;  but  without 
result.  In  the  twenty-second  week  she  had  begun  to  set  the  baby 
on  arms  and  knees  on  the  large  dining-table,  bracing'  her  with  a 
hand  against  her  feet:  the  baby,  as  usual  on  feeling  her  soles  set 
against  anything,  would  push  backward,  thus  thrusting  herself 
forward;  and  as  the  table-cover  slid  on  the  smooth  surface,  and  the 
hand  behind  her  feet  was  advanced  with  her,  she  would  thus  struggle 
rapidly  across  the  table,  grunting  toward  the  end  of  the  trip  with 
an  air  of  much  exertion.  Her  grandmother  purposed  thus  to 
encourage  the  idea  of  forward  progression,  and  associate  it  with  the 
creeping  position.  The  effect,  however,  seems  to  have  been  to 
strengthen  the  association,  already  probably  incipient,  between 
forward  motion  and  the  act  of  pushing  backward  with  the  feet : 
for  on  the  167th  day,  and  again  on  the  168th,  when  the  baby 
was  lying  on  her  stomach,  and  saw  something  before  her  that  she 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  ^>Zc> 

wished  to  get,  she  began  pushing  with  her  feet  against  empty  space, 
fretting  when  she  found  this  futile.  Once  in  struggling  toward  it, 
she  approximated  creeping  movements,  but  her  feet  and  knees 
were  so  entangled  in  her  long  skirts  that  she  accomplished  nothing. 
A  considerable  advance  in  rolling  which  took  place  this  same  week 
(twenty-fourth)  now  diverted  her  from  the  disposition  to  creep,  so 
that  though  she  was  put  into  short  clothes  the  next  week,  no 
more  efforts  at  forward  movement  appeared  for  some  time.' 

This  advance  in  rolling  seems  to  have  come  about  accidentally: 
the  baby,  in  trying  to  reach  something,  rolled  over  entirely,  from 
back  to  stomach.  I  saw  this  first  on  the  167th  day,  but  was  told  it 
had  happened  several  times  in  the  preceding  days.  By  the  168th 
day  she  was  rolling  over  purposely,  and  sometimes  quickly,  though 
at  first  it  had  been  done  slowly.  At  first,  too,  she  had  always 
caught  her  arm  under  her  when  she  turned,  and  extricated  it 
afterward  with  difficulty ;  now  she  drew  the  arm  out  easily.  The 
next  day,  the  169th  (just  twenty-four  weeks  old),  and  for  days 
following  she  rolled  over  whenever  she  was  laid  on  her  back,  some- 
times quickly,  sometimes  with  difficulty;  and  lying  on  her  stomach, 
would  lift  her  head  and  shoulders  and  look  round,  like  a  little 
lizard.     She  always  tired  soon  of  the  position,  however,  and  being 

'There  can  be  no  doubt  that  the  early  locomotor  movements  are  unduly 
delayed  by  the  baby's  skirts,  and  that  a  baby  would  profit  in  development  and 
use  of  limbs  by  much  nude  exercise.  After  creeping  is  learned,  babies  are  often 
put  into  "creepers"  (garments  of  rude  trouser  form,  devised  for  freedom  of 
movement  and  protection  from  dirt);  but  in  the  stage  of  development  that 
precedes  creeping,  when  a  great  number  of  approximate  movements  are  being 
made,  out  of  which  the  useful  locomotor  ones  will  later  be  selected,  the  legs 
are  usually  much  confined.  The  appearance  of  these  preliminary  movements 
should  be  a  signal  for  giving  the  baby  all  practicable  freedom  of  action.  Babies 
who  are  kept  always  on  a  bed  or  in  the  lap  are  put  at  a  great  disadvantage  in 
this  respect;  and  even  in  the  case  of  my  niece,  who  was  a  "floor  baby,"  the 
first  disposition  to  creep  may  have  been  lost  by  a  week's  delay  in  getting 
her  into  short  skirts.  The  mother  must  avoid  the  danger  of  cold,  even  at  cost 
of  checking  muscular  development:  but  it  seems  a  reasonable  conjecture  that 
without  the  hampering  influence  of  long  skirts  and  the  practice  of  keeping 
babies  off  the  floor,  this  primitive  quadrupedal  movement  would  appear  much 
earlier,  and  play  a  larger  part  in  the  infant's  activities,  than  it  does.  If  it  pre- 
ceded securely  balanced  sitting  (as  my  observations  indicate  that,  without  any 
artificial  check,  it  might),  the  less  natural  and  less  useful  hitching  would  never 
appear  as  a  substitute. 


334  University  of  California.  Vol.  r 

unable  to  turn  herself  back,  would  begin  to  fret.  Nevertheless,  she 
took  great  satisfaction  in  the  enlarged  freedom  of  her  movements, 
— which,  as  I  have  suggested  above,  may  have  been  the  cause  of 
the  cessation,  for  the  time,  of  efforts  to  sit  up. 

It  will  be  seen  that  at  the  close  of  the  first  half  year  the  baby 
was  fairly  well  able  to  sit  alone  (though  with  still  insecure  equi- 
librium); and  that  the  activity  of  trunk  and  limbs  was  considerable, 
and  the  tendency  toward  rolling  and  creeping  movements  clearly 
visible.  At  this  stage  of  development  Preyer's  child  made  regular 
walking  movements  when  held  with  his  soles  touching  a  flat  surface 
and  moved  gently  forward;  and  Champney's  child  made  these 
alternate  movements  very  perfectly  as  early  as  the  nineteenth  week. 
In  my  niece's  case,  there  was  no  sign  of  any  instinct  to  walk. 

Of  primitive  movements  that  might  have  some  relation  to  walk- 
ing, the  earliest  were  the  propping  with  the  knees  (p.  325),  and 
the  pushing  with  the  feet  against  a  resisting  surface  (pp.  180,  189) 
repeatedly  mentioned  above.  In  the  seventh  week  this  pushing 
against  the  foot  of  the  tub  was  so  strong  that  the  mother  could  not 
keep  the  baby  from  bumping  her  head  against  the  other  end  of  the 
tub.  In  the  twelfth  week,  when  I  seated  her  on  my  arm,  holding 
her  encircled  with  my  other  arm,  and  jumped  her  a  little,  she 
pushed  strongly  on  my  knee  with  her  feet.  Her  grandmother  at 
this  time  used  to  seat  her  on  her  palm  and  jump  her,  letting  her 
"  feel  her  feet"  in  the  same  way,  and  she  was  fond  of  the  exercise. 
In  the  twelfth  week  began  the  strong  and  straight  kicking  described 
above  (pp.  188-9),  showing  increased  power  over  the  movements  of 
the  legs.  In  the  fifth  month  the  baby  made  springing  movements 
with  her  body,  while  sitting  in  one's  lap  or  arms.  Usually  these 
appeared  to  be  expressions  of  joy  and  interest,  but  on  days  when 
they  were  very  persistent  they  continued  through  every  mood  ;  as 
on  the  153d  day,  when  the  baby  was  in  a  fretful  condition,  crying 
hard  unless  constantly  diverted,  yet  kept  springing  in  my  arms  as 
I  carried  her,  or  on  the  table  when  I  set  her  down  on  it.  It  did 
not  seem  to  be  in  such  cases  an  expression  of  restlessness,  but 
rather  a  persistent  and  unaccountable  impulse,  independent  of  her 
mood.  With  the  sixth  month  she  began  to  spring,  instead  of 
merely  pushing,  when    held    erect    with  her   soles    touching   any 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  335 

surface  (even  when  taken  into  the  lap):  she  would  push  down  with 
her  feet,  and  spring  so  strongly  that  it  was  hard  to  hold  her.  In 
the  latter  part  of  the  sixth  month  the  springing  sometimes  indi- 
cated desire  for  a  frolic :  at  the  same  time,  it  ceased  to  be  invariable 
when  the  baby  was  held  with  soles  touching,  —  the  old  pushing, 
or  an  aimless  kicking  and  shoving  (with  toes  turned  in),  taking  its 
place.  In  the  bath  (twenty-fifth  week)  the  baby  would  plant  her 
feet  against  the  bottom  of  the  tub  as  she  was  put  in,  before  she 
could  be  seated,  and  would  thrust  with  her  feet  till  she  lifted  herself 
almost  to  a  standing  position  ;  she  could  with  difficulty  be  made  to 
sit  down.  There  was  no  appearance  in  this  of  desire  to  stand;  it 
seemed  merely  an  impulse  to  push. 

Outside  of  this  pushing,  kicking,  and  springing,  and  a  little 
propping  of  the  body  with  the  knees,  there  was  in  the  first  half 
year  no  disposition  to  use  the  legs  in  any  way. 

Seventh    and    Eighth    Months:    Secure    Sitting;    Raising    Self 

to  Sitting  Position;    Rolling,  and  First  Creeping 

Movements;  Beginning  of  Standing. 

With  the  beginning  of  the  second  half  year  the  baby's  equilib- 
rium in  sitting  rapidly  grew  secure:  by  the  middle  of  the  seventh 
month  I  have  record  of  her  reaching  in  every  direction  as  she  sat, 
without  losing  balance,  and  by  the  end  of  the  month  she  would 
sit  alone  by  the  half  hour.  Yet  for  several  months  longer  she  was 
not  absolutely  secure  against  an  occasional  unexpected  and  appar- 
ently causeless  over-tilting,  —  perhaps  once  or  twice  a  month.  In 
the  latter  part  of  the  seventh  month  she  made  a  few  efforts  again 
to  raise  herself  directly  from  her  back ;  and  once  or  twice  pulled 
herself  up  by  some  neighboring  object. 

At  the  beginning  of  the  month,  a  little  tendency  toward  creep- 
ing again  appeared,  through  changes  of  position  and  movement 
when  she  had  turned  over  face  downward  and  propped  herself  with 
her  knees;  but  it  was  again  checked  by  an  advance  in  rolling,  — 
the  discovery  that  she  could  roll  over  from  her  stomach  to  her 
back.  This  ability  made  her  happy  in  rolling  and  kicking  by  the 
hour,  changing  her  position  at  will.  She  made  no  effort  to  pro- 
gress by  rolling,  but  if  she  happened  to  turn  over  in  the  same 


33&  University  of    California.  [Vol.  i_ 

direction  a  number  of  times  successively,  she  would  traverse  half 
the  width  of  the  room,  coming  in  contact  with  many  articles  of 
great  interest  to  her.  Sometimes  she  would  roll  over  to  reach 
something,  sometimes  merely  for  change  of  position.  Once  (at 
twenty-nine  weeks  old)  chancing  to  roll  under  a  chair,  she  laid  hold 
of  it,  and  worked  and  drew  herself  under  it,  —  doubtless  by  mere 
purposeless  pulling.  Her  delight  in  the  enlarged  freedom  of  move- 
ment not  only,  as  I  have  just  said,  stopped  entirely  the  progress 
toward  creeping,  but  considerably  suspended  the  great  pleasure 
she  had  taken  the  month  before  in  grasping. 

In  the  first  week  of  the  eighth  month  (217th  day)  the  baby 
waked  and  cried  some  time  before  she  was  heard ;  and  when  her 
mother  went  to  her,  she  found  her  sitting  up  in  bed:  whether  she 
had  pulled  herself  up  by  the  bedclothes  or  lifted  herself  from  the 
pillow  we  could  not  tell.  In  the  thirty-third  week,  I  saw  her  raise 
herself  from  a  pillow;  and  in  the  same  week,  she  would  raise  her- 
self easily  by  my  fingers  (if  started  by  a  little  suggestive  pull), 
scarcely  bringing  any  perceptible  weight  on  them.  Just  at  the  end 
of  the  month,  sitting  in  a  large  chair,  her  face  toward  its  back,  she 
chanced  to  thrust  her  feet  through  an  opening  between  its  seat  and 
back,  and  her  feet  being  thus  held,  she  began  to  lie  back  and  sit  up 
with  perfect  ease.  This  was  repeated  on  the  next  day.1  For  about 
a  week,  however,  as  she  played  about  the  floor,  she  had  had  a  more 
practical  method  of  sitting  up  than  either  pulling  up  by  some 
article  or  directly  lifting  herself  by  the  abdominal  muscles:  this  was 
to  rise  not  from  her  back  but  from  her  side,  leaning  on  one  elbow 
and  hand. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  month  she  would  sit  in  a  rocking-chair 
and  jiggle  it,  without  disturbance  to  her  equilibrium.  Her  sitting 
of  course,  even  in  a  chair,  was  not  the  customary  adult  position, 
with  knees  bent,  but  the  primitive  one,  with  legs  extended  before 

'From  the  ease  with  which  she  sat  up  it  was  evident  that  the  abdominal 
muscles  had  long  been  able  to  raise  the  trunk,  and  the  only  difficulty  had  come 
from  inability  to  keep  the  legs  down  and  give  the  lifting  muscles  leverage. 
Even  grown  people,  in  raising  themselves  by  the  abdominal  muscles,  are  obliged 
to  overcome  the  superior  weight  of  the  trunk  as  compared  to  the  legs;  and 
a  baby's  legs  are  much  lighter,  proportionally  to  the  trunk,  than  an  adult's. 


Shinn.]  The   Development  of  a    Child.  jj"/ 

her.  She  had  sat  on  the  lap  or  on  an  arm,  however,  with  knees 
bent ;  and  she  took  up  this  artificial  civilized  position  with  great 
ease,  —  though  it  is  likely  that  for  some  years  the  primitive  one 
was  somewhat  easier. 

Her  rolling  was  still  aimless,  but  more  and  more  free  and  quick. 
On  the  day  she  was  thirty-three  weeks  old  I  find  mention  of  her 
"whirling"  away,  when  her  mother  went  to  take  her  up  from  the 
table,  where  she  had  been  laid  down  and  allowed  to  roll;  and  on 
the  next  day  of  her  "singularly  rapid"  rolling.  She  would  now 
roll  over  and  over  several  times  in  rapid  succession,  apparently  for 
fun,  without  any  appearance  of  effort,  keeping  her  arms  skilfully 
out  of  the  way.  Her  joy  in  it  was  remarkable :  during  much  of 
the  month  there  was  nothing  she  so  desired  as  to  be  put  down  on 
the  floor  and  allowed  to  roll,  kick,  and  twist  about  freely.  Out- 
doors she  would  coax  to  be  put  down  instead  of  being  carried 
about.  I  have  not  noted  and  do  not  remember  that  she  ever 
wearied  of  it:  the  end  of  her  hours  of  rolling  about  would  be  set 
by  the  needs  of  feeding  or  otherwise  attending  to  her,  not  by  her 
own  fatigue. 

When  lying  on  the  fl'jor,  she  would  twist  her  body  about  very 
skilfully  to  reach  things;  and  once  (234th  da)-),  when  she  had  rolled 
partly  under  a  chair,  she  took  hold  of  it  and  pulled  it  above  her 
and  played  with  it,  greatly  pleased  with  the  performance. 

Up  to  the  middle  of  the  month  I  still  saw  no  tendency  toward 
creeping:  she  was  more  than  content  with  the  rolling.  At  thirty- 
two  weeks  old  I  twice  saw  her  rest  on  her  hands  and  knees  and 
make  two  or  three  creeping  movements,  then  sag  down  and  roll 
over.  Watching  closely  for  the  next  few  days,  I  saw  that  when 
rolling  and  twisting  on  the  floor,  she  now  and  then  raised  herself 
on  her  hands  and  knees,  but  had  no  idea  of  locomotion.  If  I  put 
my  hand  under  her,  she  was  disposed  to  lift  her  knees,  and  rest  on 
her  hands  and  feet  instead. 

Throughout  the  thirty-third  and  thirty-fourth  weeks,  it  was 
my  daily  note  that  she  would  several  times  a  day  get  to  her  hands 
and  knees,  now  and  then  even  taking  a  step  or  two  with  them  as 
if  by  accident:  the  movement,  being  one  to  which  the  position  and 


jj*5  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

the  structure  of  the  limbs  were  well  adapted,  was  one  quite  possi- 
ble to  make  almost  accidentally.  One  day  in  the  thirty-third 
week  (229th  day)  she  drew  herself  forward  flat  on  her  stomach  a 
few  inches  to  reach  something,  —  the  first  real  voluntary  locomo- 
tion. In  the  thirty-fourth  week  these  positions  were  taken  with 
more  ease  daily,  and  began  to  vary  the  rolling  about.  At  last,  on 
the  day  she  was  eight  months  old,  the  baby  half  crept,  half 
sprawled  forward  a  few  inches  to  get  something. 

During  the  same  fortnight,  she  had  been  learning  to  pull  her- 
self to  her  feet  and  stand  by  a  support.  For  about  six  weeks  in 
the  second  half  year  there  had  been  absolutely  no  progress  toward 
standing  or  walking  (except  that  an  increasing  vigor  of  back  and 
legs  was  shown  at  the  end  of  the  seventh  month,  when  the  mother 
first  let  her  sit  in  the  bath  without  a  steadying  hand :  she  sat  care- 
fully, but  celebrated  her  freedom  by  beginning  at  once  to  splash 
with  her  arms  in  high  glee ;  and  as  soon  as  her  mother,  afraid 
she  would  upset  herself,  put  a  steadying  hand  in  the  lightest  way 
behind  her  head,  the  baby  would  begin  to  stiffen  and  throw  her 
body,  resting  on  her  feet  and  her  mother's  hand).  But  at  thirty- 
two  weeks  old,  the  day  that  I  first  saw  creeping  movements,  I  noted 
that  the  baby  was  pulling  herself  to  her  knees  in  the  bath,  holding 
by  the  side  of  the  tub ;  sometimes  even  partly  to  her  feet,  sup- 
ported by  the  water.  At  thirty-four  weeks  old,  she  began  to  pull 
herself  quite  to  her  feet,  and  afterward  did  this  daily.  She  would 
hold  by  the  edge  of  the  tub,  leaning  far  forward,  her  feet  planted 
wide  apart  on  the  opposite  side  of  the  tub  (an  ordinary  wooden 
wash-tub,  her  own  little  tin  bath  having  been  outgrown);  on  the 
last  day  of  the  month,  she  brought  her  feet  nearer  together. 

Her  expression  of  pleasure  in  this  achievement  was  noticeable ; 
and  indeed  the  whole  process  of  learning  to  stand  was  more  con- 
scious than  creeping,  which  seemed  to  have  been  almost  stumbled 
upon  in  the  first  place,  and  afterward  used  merely  to  help  to  the 
attainment  of  desires;  while  the  impulse  to  pull  up  to  knees  and 
feet  seemed  to  come  on  her  in  a  curiously  instinctive  and  com- 
pelling way,  and  to  give  joy  in  the  satisfying  of  an  internal  necessity. 

In  the  thirty-fourth  week,  after  she  had  been  pulling  to  her 
knees,  and  nearly  to  her  feet,  for  a  week  or  more,  her  grandmother 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  'Child.  339 

advised  letting  her  practice  pulling  up  by  our  hands:  she  would 
sit  in  our  laps,  bracing  her  feet  against  our  bodies,  and  when 
started  by  a  slight  pull  would  draw  herself  up  till  she  was  poised 
on  her  feet,  when  we  would  at  once  bring  her  forward  to  rest  on 
the  breast,  that  she  might  not  bear  her  weight  on  her  feet  for  more 
than  a  point  of  time.  She  enjoyed  this  greatly,  and  seemed  strong 
and  perfectly  able  to  do  it;  but  in  a  day  or  two,  having  a  cold,  she 
seemed  less  able,  and  her  knees  would  bend  ;  so  it  was  given  up. 

It  was  at  this  time  that  when  taken  under  the  arms  and  swung 
out,  she  would  lift  her  body  and  legs  and  straighten  them  by  sheer 
muscle,  till  she  was  horizontal  or  more  (p.  191),  keep  the  position 
a  second  or  two,  then  relax  and  repeat  it,  —  showing  a  consider- 
able strength  of  back.  At  the  same  period,  she  would  fairly  fling 
herself  (from  a  chair,  e.  g.,)  toward  any  person  by  whom  she 
wished  to  be  taken. 

Ninth  Month:  Raising  Self  to  Sitting  Position;  Progress  in 

Creeping;    Varied  Scrambling  about;    Progress 

in  Standing;  First  Walking  Movements. 

With  the  ninth  month  began  a  period  of  rapid  and  varied 
development  in  movements,  difficult  to  narrate  in  clear  order. 

On  the  first  day  of  the  month  I  took  careful  note  of  the  baby's 

method  of  sitting  up.     When  laid  on  her  back  she  would  soon  roll 

over;  then  raise  herself  to  hands  and  knees;  then  lean  sidewise  till 

she  was  in  a  sort  of  inclined  sifting  position,  resting  on  one  hand; 

then  lift  herself  up  sitting,  —  all  quite  easily  and  unconsciously. 

The  next  day  I  saw  it  done  in  the  same  way,  but  also  by  another 

method,  which  soon  became  the  regular  one:  the  baby  would  first 

rise  to  hands  and  knees,  then  separate  the  knees  and  lift  herself 

backward,  —  a  process  that  has  been  found  practically  impossible  by 

adults  who  have  tried  it  for  me.     It  brought  the  baby  to  an  unusual 

sitting  position,  with  legs  spread  wide  before  her,  turning  out  at 

each  knee  in  a  right  angle.     I  had  watched  for  the  simian  position 

in  sitting,  so  common  with  babies,  —  legs  extended  before  them 

and  soles  turned  toward  each  other,  —  but  I  never  saw  it;  instead 
23 


34°  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

the  one  just  described,  with  the  outward  turn  at  the  knee,  became 
the  invariable  one.1 

Watching  her  a  long  time,  her  grandmother  and  I  were  both 
convinced  that  the  sitting  up  was  not  a  voluntarily  adaptive  move- 
ment, —  that  the  baby  did  not  know  whether  she  was  going  to 
bring  up  sitting  or  not,  but  tumbled  and  scrambled  around,  and 
found  herself  in  these  positions.  Apparently  she  desired  to  sit  up, 
but  had  no  clear  idea  of  the  movements  necessary,  and  experimented 
till  she  found  herself  sitting,  scolding  all  the  time.  This  seemed 
true  to  me  for  several  days,  but  the  movements  daily  became  more 
definite  and  purposive.  The  attempt  to  lift  herself  directly  from  her 
back  by  main  strength  was  entirely  given  up. 

She  would  now  sit  on  the  arm  of  a  stuffed  chair,  her  feet  in  the 
seat,  with  entire  security  of  balance;  in  the  thirty-eighth  week  she 
enjoyed  sitting  on  the  edge  of  a  hammock  and  dangling  her  legs. 

Rolling  declined  as  the  baby  became  able  to  sit  up  and  lie  down 
at  will,  and  to  occupy  herself  with  the  more  varied  movements 
about  to  be  described;  it  was  never  common  after  the  first  week  of 
this  month. 

The  scrambling  about  the  floor,  however,  became  more  varied 
and  interesting.  On  the  first  day  of  the  month,  among  sundry 
indefinite  movements,  several  steps  of  "crawfishing  "  were  taken, 
—  a  sort  of  retrograde  creeping,  pushing  backward  with  the  hands. 
Apparently  there  was  no  intention  of  getting  anywhere  by  this, 
only  a  disposition  to  movement.  On  the  second  day  the  baby 
backed  away  from  me  when  she  wished  to  come  to  me,  scolding 
with  disappointment  all  the  time.  At  other  times  she  would  back 
into  the  wall,  to  her  own  displeasure,  and  scold  vigorously,  all  the 
time  pushing  back  against  the  obstacle  like  a  little  engine  unable  to 
reverse  itself.  These  incidents  seemed  as  if  there  was  now  some 
desire  of  locomotion,  but  an  entire  inability  to  guide  or  adapt  her 
movements.  The  superior  strength  and  importance  of  the  arms  as 
compared  to  the  legs  was  evident:  the  disposition  to  use  a  simple 

'The  position  was  puzzling  to  me,  but  Dr.  Le  Conte  explains  it  as  due  to 
an  extreme  freedom  of  movement  in  the  hip-joint,  not  impossible  in  an  adult, 
though'doubtless  unusual.  Even  at  seven  years  old,  the  child  shows  a  certain 
tendency  to  turn  the  leg  outward  at  the  knee  in  sitting  flat  on  the  floor. 


Shisn.)  Tlit  Develop  incut  of  a   Child.  34 r 

pushing  movement  with  the  arms  probably  gave  the  backward 
motion,  and  the  legs  did  little  more  than  yield  to  this.  In  a  day  or 
two  the  baby's  action  in  this  "crawfishing"  became  quite  free,  and 
she  would  move  some  distance,  "half  creeping,  half  wriggling" 
(thirty-sixth  -week).  In  this  week  she  would  occasionally  take  a 
forward  step  or  two  on  hands  and  knees,  but  with  no  idea  of 
locomotion.  In  the  thirty-seventh  week,  when  sitting,  instead  of 
getting  on  hands  and  knees  to  creep  to  an  object  she  desired,  she 
would  throw  herself  prone,  stretch  her  arm  to  it,  and  sit  up  again. 
Late  in  the  week,  however,  on  the  258th  day,  real  creeping 
began.  She  several  times  crept  a  foot  or  two  forward,  and  once  I 
saw  her  rolling  an  orange  about  the  floor  and  creeping  after  it.  I 
then  placed  her  playthings  before  her,  some  four  feet  away.  She 
looked  earnestly  at  them,  but  made  no  effort  to  reach  them  till  I 
drew  them  to  within  two  feet;  then  she  crept  three  or  four  steps, 
then  dropped  prone  and  stretched  her  arm  till  she  managed  to  get 
hold  of  one.  Later  in  the  day  I  called  her  to  come  to  me,  holding 
my  arms  and  coaxing:  she  crept  about  a  foot  and  a  half,  using  her 
hands  well  but  her  knees  awkwardly  and  far  apart;  then  gave  up 
and  sat  back  on  her  heels,  calling  to  me  to  take  her,  and  would  not 
move  on  for  any  coaxing;  but  when  I  leaned  forward  and  shortened 
the  distance,  she  took  courage,  and  crept  on  till  she  could  grasp  my 
hand.  She  now  seemed  to  understand  that  she  could  get  some- 
where by  creeping,  and  daily  improved  in  it :  "  creeps  several  feet 
easily,"  I  note  the  next  day ;  the  next,  she  twice  crept  half  way 
across  the  room  to  a  door,  hearing  her  grandfather's  voice  on  the 
other  side ;  but  for  many  weeks  she  would  creep  only  short  dis- 
tances, and  always  for  an  object  of  desire,  not  for  the  pleasure  of 
the  movement. 

From  the  first  of  the  month,  she  would  sometimes  rise  from 
hands  and  knees  to  hands  and  feet,  and  then  would  strain  up- 
ward, as  if  trying  to  rise  to  her  feet.  Meanwhile,  pulling  to  the 
feet  by  some  hold  above  her  had  increased:  she  would  climb 
up  to  get  at  her  mother's  face  and  ear,  while  undressed  in  her  lap  ; 
when  heid  up  a  little  in  my  lap  she  pulled  to  her  feet  and  stood 
resting  her  weight  partly  on  them,  partly  on  her  hands  on  my 
shoulders,  much  as  she  was  in  the  habit  of  standing  in  the  bath-tub; 


342  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

held  against  her  mother's  shoulder,  she  would  pull  back,  and  try  to 
stand  in  her  lap ;  in  the  bath  her  one  idea,  as  soon  as  she  was 
placed  in  the  tub,  was  to  pull  herself  to  her  feet.  On  the  246th 
day  she  drew  herself  up  by  her  mother's  dress,  with  much  wabbling 
of  the  knees.  Later  the  same  day  she  could  not  get  to  her  feet  in 
the  bath  when  impeded  by  a  sponge  in  her  hand,  but  tried  persist- 
ently till  her  mother  took  the  sponge  away,  when  she  succeeded ; 
afterward  she  held  to  the  edge  of  the  bath  with  one  hand  and 
reached  with  the  other  for  the  sponge,  keeping  her  feet;  but  twice 
at  other  times  she  fell,  receiving  a  slight  bump  with  unconcern.  In 
the  thirty-sixth  week  her  skill  in  pulling  up  and  balancing  was 
visibly  improved  ;  once  she  drew  herself  up  by  the  edge  of  a  basket 
into  which  she  was  set,  and  once  by  a  box  in  which  her  playthings 
were  kept,  leaning  over  it  (about  a  foot  high)  and  holding  with  one 
hand,  while  she  extracted  what  she  wanted  with  the  other.  In  all 
these  cases,  except  the  one  instance  of  getting  up  by  her  mother's 
dress,  she  used  an  object  lower  than  her  standing  height,  {lifting  her 
body,  rather  than  pulling  up  by  her  arms,)  and  so  did  not  stand 
erect,  but  leaning  over,  resting  much  of  her  weight  on  her  hands  ; 
and  this,  with  the  wide  planting  of  her  feet,  made  balancing  easy. 
Unlike  sitting  and  creeping,  this  standing  was  quite  consciously 
achieved,  and  the  baby  showed  triumph  and  joy  every  time  she 
succeeded  in  getting  on  her  feet. 

She  was  now,  in  the  middle  of  the  ninth  month,  capable  of 
a  medley  of  movements  that  gave  her  singular  joy.  She  would 
tumble  about  the  floor  or  lawn,  sitting  up  and  lying  down,  raising 
herself  to  her  knees  and  thence  sitting  back,  raising  herself  to 
hands  and  knees,  then  to  hands  and  feet,  then  settling  down  again 
to  hands  and  knees;  twice  I  saw  her  (257th  day)  raise  herself  to 
her  feet  and  hands,  and  then  raise  one  hand  into  the  air  and  hold 
it  so  for  some  seconds,  —  a  position  very  suggestive  of  rising 
directly  to  her  feet.  She  would  raise  herself  from  hands  and  feet 
to  her  knees,  or  to  one  knee  and  one  foot;  she  would  sit  on  her 
heels;  or  on  one  heel,  the  other  leg  stretched  out  sidewise,  so 
that  she  propped  herself  with  the  foot.  Once  or  twice  when  she 
was  supported  on  hands  and  feet,  raising  her  body  as  high  as 
possible,  she  held  her  head  very  low,  almost  touching  the  carpet. 
I  thought  for  some  days  that  in  creeping  she  used  one  foot  and 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  343 

one  knee;  but  when  I  had  an  opportunity  to  see  her  motions  with- 
out any  skirts  (260th  day)  I  noted  that  the  leg  movements  were 
correct  in  creeping,  except  when  now  and  then,  after  she  had  just 
been  kneeling  on  one  foot  and  one  knee,  she  would  drop  down 
and  begin  to  creep,  in  which  case  she  would  for  a  step  or  two 
use  one  knee  only,  dragging  the  other  leg.  Her  kneeling  did 
not  seem  to  me  quite  regular;  the  legs  seemed  doubled  up  and 
awkward,  but  I  could  not  make  out  just  how:  she  could  not  keep 
the  position  many  seconds,  but  would  take  it  every  few  minutes, 
as  she  sprawled,  rolled,  crept,  and  sat  about  the  floor;  and  she 
appeared  to  enjoy  getting  up  to  her  knees  as  a  daring  achieve- 
ment, and  would  pat  and  wave  her  hands  exultantly  as  she 
balanced  thus.  When  creeping  she  would  often  stop  and  sit 
back,  bringing  herself  to  the  sitting  position  described  above. 

In  spite  of  her  joy  in  her  movements,  I  was  constantly 
impressed  by  the  lack  of  adaptiveness  and  intelligence  in  them, 
even  while  to  the  observer  they  were  plainly  working  slowly 
toward  our  own  well-adapted  means  of  locomotion.  One  day 
iii  the  thirty-seventh  week,  for  instance,  (255th  day)  she  had 
backed  herself  partly  under  a  lounge,  and  there  tried  to  rise  from 
hands  and  knees  to  hands  and  feet:  the  lounge  prevented,  and 
she  was  disappointed  at  her  failure,  and  tried  over  and  over  to 
rise,  but  had  not  sense  enough  to  move  forward  clear  of  the 
obstacle;  nor  did  it  occur  to  her  to  look  back  and  see  what  held 
her  down.1  There  was  so  little  skill  of  movement,  too,  that  as 
late  as  the  last  day  of  the  thirty-eighth  week,  she  slipped  in 
creeping,  and  fell  on  her  face,  hurting  her  lip. 

Thus  far  (thirty-seventh  week)  standing  did  not  enter  much 
into  the  medley  of  movements,  as  the  baby  had  not  yet  begun 
to  get  to  her  feet  except  by  the  help  of  a  few  specially  favorable 

'Yet  she  had  looked  to  see  what  touched  the  back  of  her  head  as  early 
as  the  fifth  month  (p.  143);  and  the  last  day  of  the  eighth  month  (243d)  she 
had  showed  a  good  deal  of  adaptiveness.  She  had  chanced  to  roll  under  her 
crib,  and  to  roll  and  sprawl  out  on  the  other  side,  where  she  sat  up,  with  some 
help  from  the  edge  of  the  crib,  dropping  a  plaything  in  the  effort;  it  rolled 
under  the  crib  and  the  baby  tried  to  lean  forward  and  get  it,  and  bumped  her 
forehead;  she  drew  back,  then  repeated  the  effort,  but  after  several  raps,  at 
last  bent  her  head  over  to  one  side  almost  to  her  lap,  and  then  leaned  under 
and  reached  the  article,  and  drew  back  in  the  same  way,  keeping  her  head 
carefully  lowered  till  it  was  clear  of  the  crib.  This  was  unusual  cleverness, 
however. 


344  University  of  California.  Vol.  i. 

objects;  but  the  desire  to  stand  and  skill  in  standing  grew  stead- 
ily, and  on  the  last  day  of  this  week  she  twice  drew  herself  to 
her  feet  by  a  chair,  her  knees  shaking,  to  reach  a  favorite  stuffed 
toy  cat  that  stood  on  it;  and  after  this,  pulling  up  by  chairs 
increased.  In  the  thirty-eighth  week,  as  she  played  about  the 
lawn  while  we  sat  by  with  books  or  work,  she  interspersed  her 
varied  scrambling  about  with  visits  to  us,  to  pull  to  her  feet  by 
chair  or  knee.  What  with  the  new  freedom  of  movement,  and 
the  many  objects  of  interest  on  the  lawn,  and  the  usual  diffused 
joy  in  outdoors,  her  happiness  at  such  times  was  indescribable. 
The  joyous  expansion  of  powers  experienced  by  a  baby  who  in 
one  month  has  achieved  the  power  of  creeping,  standing,  and 
various  minor  movements,  is  something  we  can  but  dimly  imagine 
from  the  exhilaration  of  acquiring  a  single  new  power  of  balance 
and  motion,  as  in  wheeling  or  swimming.  The  change  in  this 
baby's  own  consciousness  within  three  weeks  must  have  been 
enormous. 

On  the  267th  day,  the  last  of  the  thirty-eighth  week,  some 
one  looked  up  from  dinner  to  see  the  baby  standing  by  a  lounge, 
merely  steadied  by  one  hand  pressed  against  it,  while  she  waved 
the  other  with  joy  and  pride.  Her  father  sprang  and  caught  her 
as  she  went  down;  then  set  her  on  her  feet,  within  the  circle  of 
his  arms  but  without  support,  for  a  few  seconds:  her  legs  shook, 
but  she  stood  without  fear  and  with  evident  exhilaration. 

This  month  at  last  (in  the  thirty-sixth  week)  appeared  walking 
movements  when  the  baby  was  supported  from  above,  with  her 
soles  lightly  touching,  —  awkward  and  merely  approximate  move- 
ments at  first,  but  fairly  good  in  the  thirty-seventh  week.  They 
continued  whenever  the  opportunity  was  given,  and  afforded  the 
child  great  pleasure. 

Tenth    Month:    Rapid    and    Free    Creeping;    Standing    Alone; 
Stepping   by   a  Support;   Beginning  of  Climbing. 

The  last  two  days  of  the  ninth  month,  and  for  three  days  in  the 
tenth,  I  was  absent  from  home:  and  when  I  returned,  I  found  the 
baby  creeping  rapidly  and  easily,  and  pulling  to  her  feet  persistently 


Shinn.1  The  Development  of  a  Child.  345 

by  chairs.  The  varied  scrambling  about  had  disappeared,  a  sur- 
vival of  the  fittest  having  taken  place  in  favor  of  creeping  and 
standing.  She  laughed  with  pleasure  as  she  crept;  and  her  desire 
to  stand  was  incessant  and  absorbing.  Her  toys  were  neglected; 
she  was  impatient  of  being  held  in  arms,  so  eager  was  she  to  get 
to  the  floor  and  exercise  her  new  powers.  When  laid  on  a  blanket 
on  veranda  or  lawn,  she  would  at  once  start  rapidly  off  creeping 
across  tiles  or  grass,  stopping  to  investigate  plants,  dead  leaves,  or 
gravel. 

She  was  clever  and  bold  in  getting  to  her  feet,  and  would  hold 
with  only  one  hand,  even  leaning  down  sidewise  to  pick  up  some- 
thing, without  losing  balance.  On  the  278th  day  she  climbed  to 
her  feet  by  a  window-sill  while  holding  a  spool  in  each  hand;  and 
by  the  end  of  the  week  (the  fortieth)  it  was  common  enough  for 
her  to  pull  herself  to  her  feet  while  her  hands  were  occupied  with 
some  object.  She  would  creep,  also,  with  her  hands  occupied,  or 
while  gesturing  with  one  hand. 

When  standing  by  a  chair  she  would  let  go,  —  in  the  effort  to 
stand  alone,  I  thought  at  first;  but  by  the  second  day  after  my 
return  I  made  up  my  mind  that  it  was  done  for  the  fun  of  falling: 
she  would  pull  to  her  feet,  laughing;  then  deliberately  let  go  and 
come  down  sitting  with  a  thud,  and  look  up  laughing  and  trium- 
phant. This  amusing  performance  was  kept  up  for  several  days. 
The  sitting  down  was  apparently  in  her  mind  as  much  a  part  of 
the  exploit  as  the  getting  up,  and  the  force  with  which  she  came 
down  did  not  seem  to  frighten  her  in  the  least  (see  p.  205). 

On  the  279th  day,  for  the  first  time,  (except  in  the  one  instance 
when  her  father  set  her  on  her  feet,)  she  repeatedly  stood  quite 
alone  for  several  seconds.  This  she  did  not  by  letting  go  her  hold 
when  already  standing,  but  by  raising  herself  to  her  feet  with  the 
help  of  a  low  body  (her  uncle's  leg  as  he  sat  flat  on  the  lawn,  e.  g.) 
and  then,  instead  of  remaining  in  a  stooping  position,  as  she  had 
always  done  before  when  she  had  risen  to  her  feet  with  the  help  of 
a  low  support,  she  straightened  up,  abandoning  her  support,  and 
stood  quite  alone,  showing  great  pleasure  in  the  ability.  In  the 
next  two  or  three  days,  by  the  beginning  of  the  forty-first  week, 
she  proved  able  to  stand  a  minute  at  a  time  when  set  in  a  corner, 
back  to  the  wall,  so  that  the  pressure  of  her  shoulders  against  the 


346  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

wall  steadied  her;  while  she  could  stand  for  many  seconds  with 
but  the  slightest  hold,  —  as  the  fold  of  a  gown  in  her  hand,  —  and 
for  three  or  four  seconds  quite  alone. 

In  the  forty-first  week,  she  no  longer  let  go  her  hold  of  a 
support  on  purpose  to  feel  herself  drop,  but  lowered  herself  care- 
fully, and  with  a  good  deal  of  skill,  to  the  floor.  Once  (284th  day) 
when  absorbed  in  a  pretzel  she  was  sucking,  she  let  go  of  the 
chair  she  held  by,  and  stood  some  seconds  alone,  quite  absently 
and  unconsciously,  then  sat  carefully  down,  —  a  more  difficult 
thing  to  do  than  to  let  herself  down  with  the  help  of  the  chair. 
In  the  latter  part  of  the  week,  it  was  not  uncommon  for  her  to  let 
go  of  a  support  and  stand  alone  absent-mindedly,  when  interested 
in  something  she  held:  she  would  rarely  do  it  purposely  when 
standing  by  a  chair;  but  when  at  some  one's  knee,  where  she  felt 
more  confidence,  she  would  sometimes  now  let  go  in  order  to  try 
to  stand  alone  (though  her  deliberate  efforts  to  stand  alone  were 
still  usually,  as  the  week  before,  when  she  had  lifted  her  body 
erect,  thus  quitting  her  support).  She  had  incurred  bumps  from 
the  time  she  began  to  roll,  and  in  creeping  had  often  collided 
sharply  with  objects,  nor  had  falls  been  infrequent  even  thus  far  in 
standing ;  and  she  had  evidently  acquired  a  certain  amount  of 
caution.  Perhaps  this  was  in  part  instinctive,  as  in  her  refusing  to 
be  coaxed  into  creeping  over  the  edge  of  the  table,  though  our 
arms  were  beneath  to  catch  her.  Once  (284th  day),  having  failed 
to  sit  down  as  carefully  as  usual,  in  her  eagerness  to  creep  to  her 
father,  she  rolled  over  with  a  bump  that  frightened  her.  On  the 
whole,  however,  she  was  very  slightly  troubled  by  bumps  or  falls, 
and  her  caution  did  not  amount  to  timidity.  She  showed  signs  of 
fear  on  the  283d  day,  when  she  had  pulled  to  her  feet  by  a  wire 
dictionary  stand,  which  rocked  under  her  clutch;  yet  she  often 
fearlessly  pulled  up  by  rocking-chairs. 

On  the  285th  day,  she  deliberately  experimented  in  standing 
alone  as  long  as  she  could.  I  sat  on  the  grass  beside  her,  and 
she  would  help  herself  up  by  my  knee,  lift  her  body  erect,  bal- 
ancing with  outstretched  arms  in  great  glee  as  long  as  she  could, 
then  come  down  plump  on  the  grass  and  try  it  again.     I  had  not 


shinn.1  The  Development  of  a    Child.  347 

a  watch  at  hand,  but  estimated  by  counting  that  the  longest  period 
for  which  she  kept  her  balance  was  about  seven  seconds.1 

Once  in  this  week,  seeing  the  stair  door  open,  she  crept  to  it 
and  looked  upstairs,  then  got  to  her  feet  without  anything  to  hold 
to  or  rest  her  hands  on,  merely  steadying  herself  by  the  door- 
frame, and  then  turned  around  to  look  at  the  people  in  the  room, — 
a  noticeable  advance  in  equilibrium,  though,  forgetting  in  her  inter- 
est to  cling  to  the  door-frame,  she  lost  her  balance  at  once. 

I  noticed  at  this  time  —  forty-first  week  —  an  intangible 
but  great  increase  in  the  baby's  appearance  of  comprehension 
and  mental  alertness :  the  new  possession  of  herself  had  opened 
somehow  a  new  era  in  her  psychic  life.  She  often  wore  a  puz- 
zled look,  —  a  curious  drawing  out  and  straightening  at  the 
corners  of  the  mouth.  An  increased  desire  of  exploring  was 
becoming  apparent.  Before  this  she  had  investigated  objects  as 
they  came  in  her  way;  now  she  began  to  have  a  passion  (which 
lasted  and  increased  for  weeks  and  months)  to  go  and  find  what 
there  was  to  see.  Her  creeping,  which  steadily  increased  in  speed 
and  distance,  was  now  purposely  utilized  to  satisfy  her  curiosity. 
On  the  284th  day,  e.  g.,  she  watched  her  grandfather  as  he  left 
the  room;  then  after  sitting  gravely  a  moment,  crept  to  the  window 
and  climbed  up  to  look  after  him.  On  the  lawn,  she  would  leave 
her  blanket  and  make  for  the  edge  of  the  grass  to  finger  the  gravel  of 
the  walk,  or  feel  over  the  bark  of  the  trees.  She  crept  into  the  hall 
and  explored  it,  sitting  down  in  each  corner  to  survey  it,  and 
looking  up  the  walls.  She  cared  little  for  toys,  but  occupied 
herself  on  the  floor  for  hours  together  by  creeping  from  chair 
to  chair,  from  person  to  person,  pulling  up  to  her  feet,  and  setting 
herself  down  again  cautiously  and  cleverly;  usually  smiling  and 
crowing  over  her  success,  and  sometimes  coming  to  us  for  applause 
and  caresses.  She  was  unwilling  to  be  taken  up  from  the  floor 
and  put  into  her  high-chair  for  meals,  and  wished  to  take  her 
milk  standing  on  the  floor,  interspersing  doses  of  it  with  play. 
She  liked  to  creep  fast  away  from  us,  laughing,  when  she  thought 


1  It  is  useful  in  keeping  record  of  the  Instinctive  Movements,  of  Attention, 
and  perhaps  some  other  categories,  to  learn  to  count  seconds;  for  a  baby 
gives  no  notice  beforehand  when  he  is  going  to  do  something  that  vou  will 
wish  to  time,  and  one  is  often  caught  without  a  watch  ready. 


348  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

we  were  going  to  pick  her  up.  Her  overflowing  joy  on  the  lawn 
at  this  time  I  have  described  elsewhere  (p.  250-i),and  even  indoors 
her  habitual  happiness  was  notable. 

In  the  forty-second  week,  her  delight  in  standing  alone 
increased,  and  the  length  of  time  for  which  she  could  keep  her 
balance  grew.  She  continued  the  voluntary  exercises  in  standing 
beside  me  on  the  lawn,  and  once,  on  the  293d  day,  becoming 
interested  in  looking  at  something  while  on  her  feet,  kept  her 
balance  for  about  a  quarter  of  a  minute;  she  could  stand  longer 
when  her  attention  was  diverted  from  her  own  equilibrium.  When 
trying  to  recover  her  balance  after  she  had  begun  to  topple,  she 
would  use  her  arms,  circling  with  them  exactly  as  a  grown  per- 
son does  when  almost  falling;  the  movement  (as  well  as  the  mere 
extending  of  the  arms  in  balancing  noticed  the  week  before)  seems 
to  be  highly  instinctive.  When  she  finally  came  down  with  a  jolt 
she  would  break  into  a  peal  of  laughter. 

In  this  week,  she  was  fond  of  standing  for  many  minutes  at 
a  low  window  and  looking  out,  watching  for  the  dog  and  other 
objects  of  interest,  —  another  source  of  mental  stimulus  and 
pleasure,  obtainable  by  herself  independently,  which  the  power 
of  creeping  and  standing  had  opened  to  her. 

On  the  293d  day  occurred  what  may  perhaps  be  regarded 
as  the  first  climbing,  consisting  merely  in  letting  herself  down  by 
her  arms.  She  had  been  seated  in  a  large  rocking-chair,  and  had 
squirmed  about  till  she  was  lying  on  her  face,  with  her  feet  to 
the  edge,  and  then  let  herself  skilfully  down  to  the  floor. 
We  put  her  back  into  the  chair,  but  she  could  not  get  herself 
again  into  position  to  drop  down;  after  we  placed  her  in  position, 
she  did  so  very  well.  Before  this  she  was  for  a  long  time 
very  happy  in  getting  up  on  her  feet  and  exploring  the  chair- 
back,  leaning  to  look  over  it  till  the  chair  tipped,  to  her  pleasure; 
the  feeling  of  an  unstable  footing  did  not  seem  to  trouble  her  at  all. 

Kneeling,  which  had  declined  since  she  learned  to  stand,  had 
not  been  entirely  given  up.  In  the  forty-second  week  (291st  day) 
I  saw  the  baby  drop  from  her  feet  to  her  knees,  while  standing 
by  a  chair.     She  was  naked  at  the  time,  so  that  I  saw  the  move- 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  349 

ment  well,  and  the  kneeling  was  quite  perfect.  In  saying  that 
kneeling  is  not  acquired  till  long  after  standing,  Professor  Preyer 
must  mean  that  it  is  not  until  long  after  a  child  can  stand  firmly 
that  he  can  drop  without  support  from  a  standing  to  a  kneeling 
position,  —  but  this  is,  indeed,  not  quite  easy  to  a  grown  person. 
My  niece  rose  to  a  kneeling  position  before  she  could  stand,  and 
lowered  herself  to  her  knees,  with  some  support,  before  she  could 
fairly  stand  alone. 

At  the  end  of  the  forty-second  week,  the  baby  was  absent  for 
five  days  on  a  railroad  trip  with  her  parents,  and  was  of  course  a 
good  deal  in  their  arms.  On  her  return,  299th  day,  and  in  the 
few  remaining  days  of  the  tenth  month,  she  showed  some  loss 
of  disposition  to  stand  and  creep.  She  crept  for  shorter  distances, 
and  only  to  reach  some  desired  place  or  object;  and  when  the 
object  was  at  some  distance,  say  twelve  or  fifteen  feet,  though  she 
would  creep  to  it  when  no  attention  was  paid  her,  she  would 
stop  if  she  was  noticed  and  ask  to  be  carried.  She  did  not 
desire  to  be  held,  however,  and  was  happy  in  playing  about  the 
floor  with  some  one  near  by,  creeping  short  distances,  getting  to 
her  feet  and  down  again,  and  exploring  as  before,  —  especially 
in  new  rooms.     She  had  not  improved  in  standing  at  all. 

She  had,  however,  made  one  notable  advance,  —  she  proved 
able,  on  her  return,  to  step  along  by  a  chair,  holding  to  the  seat. 
I  had  been  told  of  her  doing  this  nearly  four  weeks  earlier,  but 
had  never  been  able  to  see  it;  and  if  there  was  no  mistake  about 
the  fact,  it  must  have  been  a  case  of  accidental  or  mechanical 
anticipation  of  a  power,  such  as  is  related  above  under  the 
subject  of  Grasping  (p.  307). 

Her  walking  movements  when  held  had  somewhat  improved 
during  the  month,  but  were  still  irregular,  and  she  showed  no 
great  disposition  to  make  them. 

On  the  last  day  of  the  month  occurred  what  may  perhaps 
more  properly  be  called  the  first  climbing  than  the  mere  letting 
herself  down  by  her  arms  eleven  days  earlier:  it  was,  however, 
to  a  considerable  extent  induced.  The  stairs  had  for  many  days 
in  the  latter  part  of  the  tenth  month  interested  the  baby,  and  she 


35°  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

crept  to  them  and  hovered  about  them,  —  not  so  much  as  if 
she  was  desirous  of  climbing,  as  because  they  made  a  surface  for 
investigation  yet  untried:  and  on  this  day  (the  304th)  as  she 
hovered  about  the  lower  step  some  one  helped  her  to  put  her 
knee  up  on  it.  The  baby  thereupon  pulled  herself  up  to  the 
step,  and  with  similar  help  up  two  more.  I  then  set  a  candle 
on  a  higher  stair,  and  desiring  to  reach  it,  the  baby  ascended  a 
fourth  step  without  help,  quite  cleverly. 

Eleventh    Month:    Progress  in  Standing  Alone,  Climbing,  and 
Stepping  by  a  Support. 

The  arrest  of  progress  in  standing  lasted  nearly  three  weeks  in 
all,  extending  some  ten  days  into  the  eleventh  month ;  but  there 
was  no  decline,  so  far  as  my  record  shows:  the  baby  seems  to  have 
stood  up  as  much  as  before,  and  with  conspicuous  joy  and  pride. 
Her  pretty  elation  when  set  on  her  feet  in  a  corner  is  especially 
noted  on  the  312th  day:  she  looked  from  one  to  another  for  notice, 
calling  across  the  room  to  members  of  the  family,  and  leaning  for- 
ward to  make  sure  that  each  one  saw  her.  That  she  was  able  to 
lean  forward  thus  (and  also  to  pat  her  hands)  without  losing  her 
balance,  showed  a  slowly  increasing  steadiness  on  her  feet,  even 
during  this  period  of  apparently  arrested  progress. 

During  the  same  interval  very  slight  progress  was  made  in  any 
other  movement.  She  appears  to  have  continued  to  edge  along  a 
few  steps  now  and  then  when  holding  by  a  chair,  and  after  the 
309th  day  would  step  the  length  of  a  lounge,  holding  on  with  one 
hand.  She  would  pass  from  chair  to  chair,  resting  a  hand  on  one 
and  reaching  as  far  as  possible  to  touch  the  next  one,  and  so  step 
across  to  that  with  the  slightest  steadying  touch;  but  she  ventured 
no  step  alone. 

The  disposition  to  creep  declined  somewhat,  though  when  the 
baby  wished  to  get  anything  she  could  creep  quite  rapidly.  On 
the  307th  day  she  made  quite  an  advance  in  intelligence  in  steer- 
ing, so  to  speak.  She  was  in  the  habit  of  creeping  to  me  when  I 
called  her;  but  if  I  called  when  she  was  on  the  floor  and  I  at  the 
opposite  end  of  a  table,  instead  of  creeping  to  me  she  would  creep 
to  the  table  and,  getting  to  her  feet,  would  stretch  up  to  peer  over 
it  at  me,  as  if  she  did  not  see  how  she  could  reach  me  through  the 


shins  j  filc  Development  of  a   Child.  35  I 

obstacle.  On  this  day  I  leaned  far  out,  so  that  she  could  see  me 
behind  her  mother,  who  sat  at  the  side  of  the  table,  and  then  she 
caught  the  idea  of  creeping  around  to  me.  A  week  later  I  tried  it 
again,  and  again  had  to  help  her  by  leaning  out  to  the  side;  but 
afterward  she  often  crept  around  to  me.  On  the  312th  day  she 
crept  tinder  the  table  while  the  family  sat  at  it,  to  find  her  grand- 
mother, and  often  did  so  afterward,  directing  herself  to  the  proper 
place  very  well. 

With  the  314th  day  came  a  sort  of  fresh  start  in  several  direc- 
tions, almost  like  the  expenditure  of  accumulated  force.  On  this 
day  the  baby  lifted  herself  to  her  feet  by  resting  her  hands  on  a 
stone  coping  not  six  inches  high,  —  the  first  time  that  I  had  seen 
her  raise  herself  to  stand  alone  unless  some  one  was  close  at  hand. 
The  same  day  I  set  her  on  her  feet  in  the  middle  of  the  lawn  :  she 
was  proud  and  interested,  and  kept  her  balance  about  a  quarter  of 
a  minute,  longer  than  she  had  ever  stood  while  attending  to  her 
equilibrium,  though  three  weeks  earlier  she  had  stood  as  long 
when  interested  in  something  else. 

The  next  day  (315th)  several  notable  advances  were  made  in 
standing,  climbing,  creeping,  and  walking,  —  as  well  as  in  some 
mental  activities,  not  recorded  in  this  chapter.  Much  of  the  day 
was  spent  outdoors  on  the  lawn,  where  her  great  enjoyment 
seemed  to  stimulate  activity  in  every  way.  As  she  played  about 
me  here,  I  looked  up  from  my  work  to  see  her  standing  quite 
alone  in  the  middle  of  the  lawn,  near  no  object  by  which  she 
could  have  raised  herself:  she  must  have  risen  directly  from  feet 
and  hands.  She  was  not  seen  to  do  this  again  before  the  twelfth 
month.  Once  as  I  was  preparing  a  loquat  for  her  to  eat,  she  got 
to  her  feet  holding  to  me,  and  in  her  eagerness  let  go  and  stood 
before  me  waiting  till  the  fruit  was  ready,  and  while  I  fed  it  to 
her  in  bits,  —  a  time  which  I  determined  pretty  closely  at  one 
and  a  half  minutes.  She  seemed  tired  then,  and  sank  partly  down 
to  a  kneeling  or  sitting  position,  but  at  once  rose  again  and  stood, 
steadying  herself  by  resting  a  hand  on  my  wrist,  or  grasping  my 
sleeve,  for  about  five  minutes  more.  Over  and  over  she  stood  ' 
alone  a  quarter-minute,  without  the  aid  of  diversion  of  attention 
from  the  standing.     The  same  day  I  saw   her  pull  herself  up  on 


35-  University  of  California.  Vol.  i. 

tiptoes  to  iook  over  the  seat  of  her  high-chair ;  and  after  this, 
standing  on  tiptoe  to  reach  or  see  something  is  repeatedly  men- 
tioned. In  spite  of  the  marked  improvement  in  equilibrium,  how- 
ever, she  had  an  especially  bad  upset  on  this  315th  day,  losing  hold 
and  balance  as  she  stood  by  the  lounge,  and  going  over  backward 
with  a  sharp  bump,  which  frightened  her  unusually. 

Until  to-day  there  had  been  no  progress  in  climbing  since  the 
small  and  induced  advance  made  ten  days  before:  but  now,  as  I 
sat  on  the  floor,  the  baby  put  up  one  foot  into  my  lap,  holding  to 
my  shoulder,  then  with  much  difficulty  and  many  efforts  drew  up 
the  other,  knee  first.  This  she  did  three  or  four  times.  She  also 
stepped  over  my  leg,  holding  by  my  arm,  the  first  time  she  had 
lifted  her  foot  to  set  it  over  an  obstacle. 

On  the  lawn  the  same  day,  as  she  played  beside  me,  I  set  an 
article  that  I  did  not  wish  her  to  reach  on  the  other  side  of  me : 
she  sat  and  looked  at  it  a  few  seconds,  then  turned  and  crept  back 
around  me  to  get  it ;  a  decided  advance  in  locomotor  idea  of  direc- 
tion. As  she  came  near  to  it,  I  set  it  again  on  the  other  side  of 
me,  and  this  time  she  scrambled  over  my  knees  to  it.  After  this, 
in  playing  about  me,  she  scrambled  ever  me  or  into  my  lap 
several  times. 

One  more  achievement  of  this  active  day  was  the  discovery 
that  she  could  push  a  chair  before  her,  which  she  twice  did,  for  a 
couple  of  steps. 

The  next  day,  316th,  as  the  baby  stood  alone  on  the  lawn,  she 
saw  some  one  bringing  a  much-desired  glass  of  water,  and  in  her 
eagerness  took  one  almost  unconscious  step  forward  and  promptly 
collapsed  on  the  grass. 

At  a  neighbor's,  she  occupied  herself  for  a  long  time  in  letting 
herself  down  backward  from  a  doorstep,  and  climbing  up  it  again. 

These  three  days  of  marked  revival  of  activity  brought  the 
aby  to  the  end  of  the  forty-fifth  week.  During  the  rest  of  the 
month  her  progress  in  climbing  was  the  most  striking  trait  of 
motor  development.  On  the  317th  day  she  climbed  of  her  own 
accord  up  two  steps  of  the  stairs.  Later  in  the  day  I  set  her  at  it 
again,  and  moved  on  up  the  stairs  before  her  to  induce  her  to 
follow,  though  I  did  not  urge  her  at  all.     I  wished  to  see  how  far 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  353 

she  could  go.  She  toiled  on  to  the  top  of  the  flight,  fifteen  steps, 
with  a  rise  of  6j/£  inches;  once  she  fell  backward  and  was  caught 
by  her  mother,  who  followed  behind  for  that  purpose;  and  two 
or  three  times  she  slipped  back  and  lost  a  step,  but  kept  at  it 
diligently.  She  was  greatly  pleased  to  have  accomplished  it, 
though  out  of  breath:  she  pulled  to  her  feet  by  the  post  on  the 
landing,  hesitated,  made  some  motions  to  creep  down  head  first, 
then  seeing  the  remaining  steps  across  the  landing  (five  in  number) 
crept  over  to  them  and  mounted  them.  All  the  way  up  she  occa- 
sionally grunted  with  exertion  as  she  drew  herself  up  a  step,  and 
always  uttered  a  satisfied  exclamation  on  achieving  it.  She  did 
not  use  her  knee  this  time,  but  placed  one  foot  on  the  step,  reached 
forward  and  put  her  hands  on  the  step  next  above,  and  so  pulled 
the  other  foot  up,  thus  climbing  rather  than  creeping.  She  was 
very  exultant  on  reaching  the  second  floor,  shouting  and  laughing. 
She  got  to  her  feet,  looked  longingly  at  the  garret  stairs,  and  ran 
her  eyes  up  them  to  the  landing;  but  before  she  reached  the  foot 
was  diverted  from  them  by  the  sight  of  a  room  door  open,  invit- 
ing to  exploration. 

After  this  she  climbed  upstairs  again  almost  daily,  at  first  slip- 
ping several  times,  and  getting  discouraged  and  asking  to  be  taken, 
on  the  lower  stair,  but  eager  to  go  on  after  she  reached  the  land- 
ing. By  the  latter  part  of  the  forty-sixth  week  I  note  neither  slips 
nor  discouragement.  In  the  forty-seventh  week,  she  lost  interest  in 
the  stairs  for  a  few  days,  but  before  the  end  of  the  week  seems  to 
have  returned  to  the  exercise,  and  to  have  become  skilful  in  climb- 
ing, —  perhaps  caring  for  it  now  merely  as  a  means  to  reach  the 
second  floor,  no  longer  as  a  diversion  in  itself.  She  could  go  up 
easily  and  rapidly,  rarely  slipping,  and  with  but  two  or  three 
pauses.  Twice  she  started  up  the  garret  stairs  also,  but  was 
diverted  from  her  purpose  half  way  up.  She  never  fell  backward 
after  the  first  ascent,  on  the  317th  day.  At  a  neighbor's,  on  the 
331st  day,  she  found  the  stairs,  and  climbed  up  many  times,  and  set 
off  to  explore  the  upper  rooms.  The  same  day  at  home  she  several 
times  climbed  up  five  or  six  steps,  then  turned  and  climbed  down 
quite  skilfully.  By  the  end  of  the  month,  in  the  forty-eighth  week, 
she  climbed  up  and  down  stairs  rapidly  and  easily,  without  any 
help,  and   wished  to  do  it  many  times  a  day:    she  would    crawl 


354  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

persistently  to  the  foot  of  the  stairs  and  start  up.  Her  method  of 
getting  down  was  to  slide  backward  from  step  to  step,  just  as  she 
did  with  increasing  ease  in  getting  down  from  a  lap.  She  never 
slipped,  but  as  she  was  disposed  to  stop  and  stand  alone  boldly  on 
each  step,  and  was  not  yet  secure  on  her  feet,  there  was  some  risk 
in  climbing,  and  some  one  always  kept  close  beside  her:  once  only, 
on  the  last  day  of  the  month  (the  335th  day),  she  lost  balance  in 
standing  thus  on  the  stair,  and  was  frightened,  though  I  caught  her. 
Meanwhile  a  remarkable  disposition  to  climb  was  evident  in 
other  ways.  Standing  by  a  chair,  tub,  or  other  piece  of  furniture, 
the  baby  would  put  up  one  foot  against  it,  seeking  some  higher 
support  (forty-sixth  week).  The  319th  day,  sitting  in  a  chair  by 
her  bath-tub,  which  was  supported  on  a  box,  she  set  one  foot  on  the 
edge  of  the  box  and  one  on  the  rung  of  the  chair,  and  leaned  over 
to  paddle  in  the  water,  — -  kept  from  falling  by  me,  but  resting  her 
weight  on  her  feet.  In  the  forty-seventh  week,  when  she  came  to 
any  obstacle,  she  constantly  lifted  a  foot  as  if  to  step  on  or  over  it. 
She  lifted  her  foot  thus  when  leaning  over  the  edge  of  the  box 
where  her  playthings  were  kept;  when  her  grandfather  lay  down 
on  the  floor  to  play  with  her,  she  kept  trying  to  step  up  on  him. 
She  could,  in  this  forty-seventh  week,  manage  a  step  of  six  or 
seven  inches,  with  a  good  hold  above,  as  on  the  stairs,  and  could 
creep  over  any  three  or  four  inch  obstacle.  The  last  day  of  the 
week  she  climbed  over  a  leg  that  was  put  out  as  a  barrier,  a  foot 
above  the  floor:  she  threw  her  body  against  it,  tilted  over  after 
several  attempts,  and  slid  down  head  first. 

Meanwhile  standing  was  persistent,  though  the  baby's  steadi- 
ness of  balance  improved  but  slowly.  She  was  fond  of  standing 
by  rocking-chairs  and  making  them  rock,  either  by  leaning  against 
them,  or  by  slapping  them,  which  did  not  seem  to  disturb  her 
equilibrium.  Once  (319th  day)  she  persisted  in  standing  up  in 
a  hammock,  and  refused  to  hold  to  me,  balancing,  recovering, 
tipping  against  me,  and  standing  again,  while  I  protected  her 
from  falling  as  best  I  could.  In  her  baby-carriage,  she  liked  to 
climb  to  her  feet  while  it  was  in  motion,  and  lean  far  out  to  the 
side,  or  forward  over  the  leather  strap  that  protected  her  from  falling. 
She  insisted  on  standing  to  be  wiped  after  her  bath,   instead  of 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  355 

sitting  in  her  mother's  lap.  She  rejoiced  still  in  standing  at  the 
window,  looking  out  and  laughing  at  the  sights.  The  320th  day 
she  sneezed  while  standing  alone,  without  losing  balance.  The 
323d  day  she  stood  fearlessly  by  the  wire  dictionary  holder  whose 
tremulousness  had  alarmed  her  a  few  weeks  before,  and  tried  to 
get  its  cord  off.  She  was  fond  of  getting  to  her  feet  in  our  laps 
to  play  with  our  hair,  etc.  By  the  forty-seventh  week  she  could 
stand  a  minute  or  two  quite  alone  at  any  time,  but  was  apt  to  drop 
down  sooner,  to  avoid  a  fall :  she  had  lost  her  sense  of  novelty 
and  achievement  in  balancing,  and  no  longer  tried  to  stand  as 
long  as  she  could,  but  was  constantly  standing  up  and  sitting 
down;  when  on  her  feet  for  a  long  time  she  kept  her  hand  on 
some  support,  however  slight.  At  the  beginning  of  the  forty- 
eighth  week  I  noticed  an  increased  firmness  in  her  standing,  and 
especially  when  she  was  occupied  in  reaching  for  or  holding  some 
object,  and  unconscious  of  her  equilibrium.  On  the  331st  day, 
e.  g.,  she  stood  alone  by  a  basket  of  toys,  leaned  over,  reached 
down  and  took  out  what  she  wished,  straightened  up  again,  and 
then  shook  hard  the  objects  she  held  in  both  hands,  all  without 
disturbing  her  balance. 

While  standing  showed  so  much  improvement,  and  climbing 
had  reached  a  considerable  skill  and  was  eagerly  desired,  walking 
interested  the  baby  scarcely  at  all.  She  continued  to  edge  about 
a  good  deal,  holding  to  chairs  or  lounge,  but  there  was  no  dis- 
position to  forward  stepping.  When  she  was  held  from  above, 
however,  she  would  walk  forward  with  clumsy  alternate  move- 
ments, and  we  gradually  allowed  her  to  take  more  and  more  of 
her  own  weight  in  doing  this;  but  she  showed  no  pleasure  in  it 
and  soon  would  wish  to  drop  down  again:  she  seemed  to  find  the 
movement  scarcely  more  normal  than  a  puppy  does  when  one 
leads  him  by  his  fore  paws.  The  first  day  of  the  forty-seventh 
week,  however,  standing  beside  me  as  I  sat  on  the  floor,  she  took 
my  hand  and  stepped  forward,  carrying  the  hand,  with  my  arm 
outstretched,  almost  as  far  backward  as  it  would  go.  This  was  more 
like  a  real  disposition  to  walk  than  any  she  had  hitherto  shown. 
A  few  days  later  she  began  to  push  chairs  about  a  little,  stepping 
after;  but  she  would  not  take  the  step  till  she  had  to,  leaning  far 

24 


356  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1 

forward  as  the  chair  moved:  what  she  cared  for  was  evidently 
moving  the  chair,  not  walking.  At  the  end  of  the  month  we 
found  that  she  could  walk  pretty  well  held  only  by  one  hand, 
but  she  did  not  care  to. 

Early  in  the  month  (307th  day)  I  had  noticed  a  tendency  to 
rise  imperfectly  to  hands  and  feet,  instead  of  hands  and  knees,  in 
creeping  fast.  The  tendency  increased  throughout  the  month, 
and  on  the  323d  day  I  noticed  that  every  time  the  baby  started 
off  it  was  on  hands  and  feet,  but  that  after  a  few  steps  she  would 
drop  to  hands  and  knees.  She  seemed  to  like  the  movement  on 
hands  and  feet  better,  but  to  be  unable  to  keep  it  up  as  long. 

Besides  this  rising  to  hands  and  feet,  sitting  back  on  her  heels 
remained  regularly  from  the  many  positions  connected  with  creep- 
ing in  the  ninth  month. 

An  incident  of  the  324th  day  gives  a  fair  idea  of  the  extent  of 
the  baby's  command  of  her  body  in  the  latter  part  of  the  eleventh 
month.  As  she  sat  in  my  lap  by  the  lounge,  she  saw  her  uncle's 
sun  helmet  on  the  lounge,  and  climbed  over  from  my  lap  to  the 
lounge  to  get  it,  pulled  it  down  with  trouble,  let  herself  carefully 
down  to  the  floor,  and  lost  hold  of  the  sun  helmet,  which  rolled  away. 
Some  diversion  occurred  here,  but  presently  the  baby  crept  over 
and  pulled  the  clumsy  burden  back  to  my  side,  climbed  up  half  into 
my  lap,  and  held  the  sun  helmet  up  awkwardly  for  me  to  put  on. 

Her  enjoyment  in  playing  around  and  using  her  powers  was 
still  notable :  she  was  happy  by  the  hour  exploring  from  room  to 
room  about  the  floor,  lingering  over  interesting  objects,  shouting 
and  calling,  getting  to  her  feet  by  chairs  and  down  again,  standing 
up  by  the  book  shelves  to  pull  out  books,  and  occasionally  sitting 
dow.i  with  one  to  turn  the  leaves  over  carefully,  climbing  the  stairs, 
edging  along  by  chairs  and  window-sills  and  creeping  across  gaps, 
standing  by  the  window  and  looking  out,  reaching  up  on  tiptoe 
to  examine  bureau-handles,  etc.  If  I  sat  down  on  the  floor  near 
where  she  was  playing,  she  would  creep  to  me,  and  holding  by  my 
arm  or  knee,  pull  to  her  feet  and  scramble  about  and  over  me, 
sometimes  trying  to  play  with  my  hair,  sometimes  merely  clinging 
and  scrambling,  amusing  herself  by  standing  up  and  sitting  down, 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  357 

etc.  Outdoors,  her  happiness  in  getting  about  was  still  greater, 
but  was  now  somewhat  marred  by  her  desire  to  ramble  off  for 
longer  distances  and  over  more  various  surfaces  than  could  be 
allowed,  so  that  she  would  beg  to  be  carried  in  order  to  get  away 
from  her  limits,  and  then  constantly  wish  to  be  put  down  to  creep; 
if  she  had  been  allowed,  she  would  have  made  her  way  by  creeping 
all  across  the  garden.  When  taken  out  in  the  baby-carriage,  she 
would  lean  out  at  the  side,  desiring  to  get  out  and  use  her  own 
powers. 

Twelfth  Month:  Raising  Self  to  the  Feet;  Climbing;  Beginning 
of  Walking  Alone. 

In  the  first  week  of  the  twelfth  month  the  mother  and  grand- 
mother told  me  that  the  baby  was  raising  herself  to  her  feet  in  the 
middle  of  the  floor  constantly  and  easily,  —  a  thing  that  she  must 
have  done  on  the  lawn  once,  three  weeks  earlier,  but  that  no  one 
had  seen  her  do  till  now;  and  I  did  not  see  her  do  it  for  ten  days 
more,  and  even  then  she  tipped  over  immediately.  Her  standing 
was  certainly  quite  firm:  on  the  340th  day,  e.  g.,  she  rejoiced  in 
standing  up  at  the  front  of  a  wagon  to  see  the  horses  go,  and  let 
go  of  the  dashboard  to  wave  her  hands  as  she  shouted.  She 
could  bend  down  and  recover  while  standing  (fiftieth  week);  and 
though  I  do  not  find  any  note  of  longer  standing  than  a  minute  at 
a  time,  this  was  doubtless  because  she  did  not  find  it  comfortable 
or  interesting  to  stand  still  longer,  not  because  this  was  the  limit  of 
her  ability. 

Climbing  up  and  down  stairs  was  a  delight  all  the  month.  The 
stairs  were  among  the  things  most  joyously  recognized  on  her 
return  from  a  few  days'  absence  in  the  fiftieth  week.  She  climbed 
as  before,  planting  her  feet,  and  slid  down  backward ;  sometimes 
she  amused  herself  by  climbing  up  a  few  steps,  then  sliding  rapidly 
down.  Late  in  the  month,  356th  day,  having  climbed  to  the  top  of 
the  stairs,  she  turned  and  slid  down,  sometimes  head  first,  some- 
times feet  first,  quite  recklessly;  this  time  she  might  have  fallen 
once  or  twice  had  I  not  caught  her,  but  as  a  rule  she  was  very  firm 
and  safe  in  these  movements,  and  needed  no  help  at  all,  though 
some  one  always  kept  close  by  her.     On  the  361st  day  she  started 


35$  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

to  creep  down,  and  crept  down  four  steps  perfectly  well,  once  or 
twice ;  the  next  day  she  crept  all  the  way  down,  without  help,  and 
rapidly.  In  other  ways  she  showed  steadily  and  increasingly  the 
desire  to  climb :  standing  in  the  bath-tub,  she  repeatedly  put  a  foot 
over  the  edge;  on  a  mattress  about  two  feet  high,  she  crept  to  the 
edge,  looked  over,  turned  round,  and  let  herself  down  backward. 

During  the  early  part  of  the  month  she  crept  about  half  the 
time  on  hands  and  feet,  but  not  for  many  feet  continuously ;  later, 
the  habit  declined,  but  the  decline  of  all  creeping  was  not  far  off  now. 

In  the  forty-ninth  week,  the  baby  walked  easily  when  steadied 
by  one  hand,  but  moved  her  feet  clumsily,  and  once  showed  a 
tendency  to  bring  one  foot  to  the  other,  instead  of  past.  She  now 
first  showed  a  little  pride  in  it.  On  the  343d  day  she  was  expect- 
ing to  go  upstairs  with  me,  and  as  I  led  her  toward  the  stairs  she 
walked  quite  well,  forgetting  herself  in  her  eagerness  to  go  with  me. 

Up  to  this  time,  she  had  been  wearing  little  soft  kid  moccasins, 
but  now  low  shoes  with  soles  were  put  on  her  feet  (346th  day) 
which  may  have  affected  her  walking  somewhat.     • 

The  347th  day,  while  going  from  chair  to  chair,  she  twice  let 
go  of  one  chair,  stood  hesitantly,  made  a  movement  forward,  even  a 
slight  hitch  forward  with  her  foot,  then  gave  up  the  idea  of  ventur- 
ing to  take  a  step  alone,  and  dropped  to  the  floor  and  crept,  though 
the  space  was  but  a  few  inches.  So  for  several  days  more:  she 
was  happy  by  the  hour  edging  and  creeping  about  the  room  freely, 
from  one  object  to  another;  but  she  would  not  take  a  step  alone, 
even  when  a  single  short  one  would  traverse  the  distance  she 
wished  to  go.  On  the  350th  day  her  mother  coaxed  her  once  or 
twice  into  it  (as  was  reported  to  me),  but  the  day  after  she  would 
not  do  it.  On  the  353d  day,  again,  I  was  told  that  she  had  walked 
three  or  four  steps,  and  this  time  spontaneously;  and  this  I  was 
able  to  verify  the  next  day,  when  if  she  was  set  against  the  wall 
and  told  to  walk,  she  would  step  forward.  She  did  it  with  much 
sense  of  insecurity,  tottering  and  taking  very  short  steps,  her  legs 
spreading  more  widely  at  each  one.  Once,  instead  of  catching  her, 
I  tested  how  far  she  could  go,  and  found  that  she  could  keep  her 
feet  for  six  steps.  She  did  not  give  up  and  sit  down,  but  went  on 
as  far  as  her  legs  would  carry  her,  with  visible  sense  of  achieve- 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  359 

merit,  tremulous,  pleased,  half-afraid,  half-proud;  and  when  she 
subsided  to  the  floor  at  the  seventh  step,  she  was  not  at  all 
frightened,  but  willing  to  get  up  and  go  on,  coming  down  as  before 
after  six  steps.  She  could  not  find  courage  to  try  again  for  several 
days,  however:  on  the  361st  day  she  tried  once,  but  fell  down  after 
a  single  step.  She  showed  desire  to  try  again,  creeping  back  to 
the  spot  against  the  wall  from  which  she  had  started,  and  standing 
there,  but  after  laughing,  waving  her  arms,  and  making  a  false 
start,  she  dropped  down  and  crept.  In  the  four  days  that  remained 
before  the  close  of  her  first  year,  she  made  practically  no  more 
progress  in  walking;  yet  sometimes  when  she  forgot  what  she  was 
doing,  she  would  take  a  step  or  two  to  a  desired  spot.  She  was 
really  perfectly  able  to  take  three  or  four,  from  support  to  support, 
for  she  was  strong  and  steady  on  her  feet;  but  she  seemed  to  have 
lost  her  little  access  of  interest  in  walking,  and  to  prefer  to  creep. 
It  was  curious  to  compare  her  doubtful  beginning  of  walking  in 
these  last  days  of  the  year  with  her  bold  and  free  climbing  and 
sliding  on  the  stairs. 

Thirteenth    Month :    Walking    Alone ;    Climbing ;    Decline    of 

Creeping. 

With  the  beginning  of  the  second  year,  walking  at  last  began 
to  develop  with  energy.  On  the  first  day  of  the  year  the  child 
walked  the  length  of  a  room,  to  the  foot  of  the  stairs,  (some 
eighteen  feet)  holding  lightly  to  my  hand,  and  then  walked  upstairs 
in  the  same  way.  She  stepped  up  nicely  and  easily  as  far  as  the 
landing,  then  lost  interest  in  walking,  in  her  desire  to  creep  down 
the  steps.  A  few  days  later  she  could  walk  freely  holding  to  one 
finger.  She  was  still  cautious  about  walking  quite  alone,  but 
wished  to  be  on  her  feet,  moving  about  and  amusing  herself,  all  the 
time,  and  was  restless  when  in  the  lap  or  the  baby -carriage;  and  she 
would  from  time  to  time  readily  enough  leave  a  support  for  a  few 
steps.  She  also  went  about  a  good  deal  pushing  her  little  chair, 
which  was  of  course  light  and  easy  to  move.  During  the  fifty-third 
week  there  were  some  fluctuations  in  walking  alone,  but  improve- 
ment on  the  whole;  and  on  the  first  day  of  the  fifty-fourth  week  I 
saw  her  walk  about  three  feet  alone.    She  was  now  becoming  disin- 


360  University  of    California.  IYol.  i. 

clined  to  get  down  and  creep,  and  was  disposed  to  take  some  one's 
finger  and  walk  across  the  room  instead.  She  walked  clumsily, 
spreading  her  legs  and  rolling  a  little:  but  the  rolling  gait  proved 
to  be  a  permanent  trick,  which  in  her  seventh  year  was  to  a  great 
extent  corrected  by  exercises.1 

On  the  376th  day  I  was  told  that  she  had  walked  alone  across 
a  room,  some  twelve  feet,  quite  spontaneously;  the  next  day  she 
walked  half-way  across  the  same  room  to  me,  straddling  and 
rolling,  and  making  grimaces  indicative  of  much  pride.  The  next 
day  she  walked  to  me  across  the  whole  width  of  the  room,  smiling 
and  proud,  walking  faster  and  faster  till  she  was  nearly  running, 
and  threw  herself  into  my  arms  with  laughter  and  kisses.  Walk- 
ing with  the  help  of  some  one's  hand  was  also  quite  advanced  in 
this  week  (fifty-fourth):  the  377th  day,  e.  g.,  she  took  me  by  the 
hand  and  led  me  across  two  rooms  to  the  place  where  her  bonnet 
was  kept,  then  after  getting  the  bonnet  on,  led  me  to  the  outer 
door.  At  fifty-four  weeks  old  creeping  was  almost  abandoned, 
except  when  she  was  in  a  special  hurry;  then  she  would  drop  down 
and  creep.  She  liked  to  utilize  a  chair  to  walk  by  when  one  was 
in  reach,  but  she  would  walk  alone  whenever  necessary,  and  would 
even  lift  her  arms,  or  go  through  gestures  while  walking;  she  liked 
to  walk  carrying  a  weight,  a  doll  or  book.  In  the  fifty-fifth  week 
she  walked  much  and  securely,  keeping  her  footing  over  slight 
obstacles,  or  a  rough  surface:  a  door-sill,  or  a  newspaper  lying 
on  the  floor,  e.  g.,  did  not  seem  to  trouble  her  at  all,  and  she  kept 
her  feet  without  difficulty  in  walking  over  a  tray  of  almonds  spread 
out  to  dry.  She  turned,  leaned  over  to  pick  up  objects,  dragged 
chairs  and  her  tin  tub  about,  and  otherwise  showed  security  on  her 
feet.  Having  overthrown  a  chair  once  accidentally,  she  did  it  after- 
ward on  purpose,  without  disturbing  her  own  balance  by  the 
exertion.     Often  she  partly  lost  her  balance,  and  recovered;  often 

'It  is  worth  while  to  notice  that  a  child,  even  of  healthy  and  normal  develop- 
ment, whose  strength  of  back  and  limbs  is  unquestionable,  may  have  defects 
of  carriage  long  before  these  could  have  been  produced  by  the  causes  so  pro- 
lific in  later  childhood,  (such  as  improper  clothing,  seats  of  wrong  size  or  shape, 
and  careless  habits) ;  and  that  his  natural  activity  can  not  always  be  trusted  to 
give  him  symmetry  and  grace.  The  pedagogic  importance  of  this  fact  I  shall 
speak  of  later. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  361 

she  went  down,  but  did  not  seem  to  mind.  She  crept  far  less  than 
she  walked.  She  led  people  about  wherever  she  wished  them  to 
go.  When  supported  under  the  arms  from  above,  or  even  once 
when  holding  by  her  mother's  hand,  she  walked  upstairs  taking 
alternate  steps,  like  a  grown  person.  Her  appearance  of  pride  in 
walking  had  nearly  disappeared,  and  she  took  it  as  a  matter  of 
course.  She  played  about,  trotting  round  the  room,  enjoying  her 
own  motions  and  examining  the  properties  of  everything,  —  experi- 
menting in  pushing  things  about  and  overturning  them,  coming  to 
us  and  looking  at  our  dresses,  ribbons,  etc.,  asking  the  names  of 
objects  by  an  inquiring  grunt;  looking  over  her  books;  looking  out 
of  the  window  and  calling  to  the  dog;  going  off  exploring,  and 
bringing  objects  to  us;  hanging  about  us  to  offer  kisses,  and  then 
off  to  explore  a  closet ;  and  so  on  by  the  hour. 

In  the  fifty-sixth  week,  I  noticed  that  she  dragged  one  foot  a 
little  in  walking,  but  it  seems  to  have  been  a  passing  trick.  She 
would  now,  when  supported  from  above,  walk  downstairs,  but  not 
alternately  like  grown  people,  as  she  did  in  walking  upstairs.  The 
increase  in  steadiness,  and  speed,  and  amount  of  walking  was 
surprising.  She  would  be  all  the  morning  outdoors,  toddling 
about,  exploring  the  garden  paths.  In  one  morning,  she  toddled 
along  the  paths  a  distance  that  amounted  to  over  two  hundred  feet 
(with  intervals  of  sitting  down  to  examine  the  ground),  besides 
making  several  shorter  trips.  The  space  in  garden  and  shrubbery, 
traversed  by  paths,  is  nearly  a  quarter  acre,  quite  irregularly  dis- 
posed, so  that  there  was  much  opportunity  for  rambling  up  this 
path  and  down  that.  Her  joy  in  this  independence  was  remark- 
able, and  she  could  not  bear  to  be  restricted,  but  was  always 
anxious  to  get  down  and  on  her  own  feet.  It  was  her  greatest 
interest,  and  an  inexhaustible  one.  She  would  toddle  along,  go 
down  on  the  ground  to  examine  something,  ask  its  name  if  it  was 
new  to  her,  get  up  and  trot  on  a  little  way,  then  down  again ;  and 
so  on  all  day  long.  One  day  in  this  week  she  escaped  supervision 
and  got  outdoors  by  herself,  and  was  captured  jubilant.  Once 
when  outdoors  she  evaded  her  mother,  toddled  round  to  another 
door,  climbed  upstairs  by  herself  to  me,  and  led  me  downstairs  and 
outdoors  with  her.  She  coaxed  all  day  to  be  allowed  to  go  out- 
doors, using  all  her  arts.     My  notes  on  other  subjects  are  full  of 


362  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

references  to  her  going  here  and  there  freely;  indeed,  walking  may 
be  said  to  have  been  completely  mastered  long  before  the  end  of 
the  thirteenth  month,  baby-like  as  her  gait  was. 

Now  and  then,  when  eager,  she  would  increase  her  speed  (as 
early  as  the  fifty-fifth  week)  to  a  sort  of  trot:  in  the  fifty-sixth  week 
she  began  the  play  of  running  away  from  people,  with  shouts  of 
laughter. 

She  persisted  meanwhile  in  trying  to  climb,  and  at  fifty-three 
weeks  old  succeeded  in  getting  out  of  the  little  tin  bath-tub;  she  set 
one  leg  over,  and  after  a  good  deal  of  doubt,  and  lifting  the  wrong 
one  (the  one  already  out),  at  last  got  the  other  one  over.  She  con- 
stantly and  easily  slid  down  backward  from  lounges,  and  over  steps; 
yet  I  find  occasional  references  to  her  asking  to  be  set  down.  She 
climbed  up  into  her  little  rocking-chair  (fifty-fifth  week)  and  stood 
holding  by  the  back  and  rocking  it.  She  was  as  fond  as  ever  of 
climbing  the  stairs,  and  if  she  could  not  go  outdoors,  coaxed  for 
the  stairs  as  the  next  best  thing.  Now  and  then  she  evaded  guard 
and  came  up  to  me  alone,  always  quite  safely.  Once  (fifty-fifth 
week)  she  began  to  scramble  down  headlong,  but  after  slipping  and 
rolling,  reversed  herself  and  slid  down  backward  as  usual.  At 
fifty-five  weeks  old,  she  would  slide  thus  off  a  bed,  feet  first,  but 
not  as  safely  as  from  a  lounge,  since  the  bed  was  so  high  that  she 
would  fall  if  we  did  not  catch  her.  She  liked  walking  upstairs 
better  than  climbing,  but  did  not  try  it  unless  supported  from 
above.  In  the  fifty-sixth  week,  she  had  a  trick  of  ejaculating 
constantly,  "Shush!  shush!  shush!"  as  she  climbed  the  stairs, — 
apparently  a  sort  of  vocal  accompanying  movement. 

Fourteenth  Month:    Secure  Walking;    Beginning  of  Running; 
Progress  in  Climbing. 

During  the  fourteenth  month  the  development  of  steadiness  and 
endurance  in  walking  was  considerable  :  the  child's  constant  desire 
was  to  walk  about  outdoors,  and  next  to  go  upstairs.  All  day  long 
she  would  beg  to  go  outdoors,  and  once  there,  would  patter  around, 
sitting  down  and  examining  objects,  then  up  and  on  again.     Her 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a  Child.  363 

usual  walk  was  about  55  yards  long,  —  along  the  veranda  and 
a  gravel  drive,  up  one  garden  path,  and  down  another;  but  from 
day  to  day  she  would  annex  some  new  strip  of  territory.  One  day 
in  the  fifty-seventh  week  she  made  three  trips,  of  some  140  yards, 
85  yards,  and  180  yards  respectively, —  the  third  one  after  a  nap, 
and  holding  by  my  finger.  This  was  probably  a  fair  sample  day  at 
thirteen  months  old. 

Her  speed  increased  perceptibly  during  the  month  ;  I  note  in  the 
sixtieth  week  that  she  "almost  runs"  when  eager;  and  the  expres- 
sion "trotted  about"  occurs  now  and  then  throughout  the  month. 
Her  gait  was  evidently  decidedly  advanced  beyond  the  toddling  of 
the  thirteenth  month.  Once,  when  she  was  sixty  weeks  old,  I  use 
the  word  "running"  of  her  coming  to  me,  guided  by  my  voice  in 
the  dark. 

Her  increased  security  of  balance  was  evident  from  her  ability 
(as  early  as  the  fifty-ninth  week)  to  trot  about  firmly  in  the  dark, 
without  stumbling.  She  walked  boldly  over  slight  irregularities, 
as  doorsills,  without  the  slightest  trip.  Still,  she  would  often  catch 
her  foot,  turn  too  quickly,  or  trip  herself  somehow,  and  sit  down 
suddenly;  she  rarely  fell  flat  or  took  any  hurt,  and  usually  laughed 
at  these  upsets.  Especially  in  reaching  her  arms  up,  to  express 
desire  for  something  above  her,  she  was  apt  to  sit  down  backward 
(fifty-eighth  week).  She  could  stoop  over  forward  and  handle 
things  on  the  floor,  sometimes  for  many  minutes  (fifty-seventh 
week)  with  ease  and  security;  and  could  lean  far  forward  (fifty- 
eighth  week),  to  look  through  a  door,  e.  g.,  without  any  risk  to 
balance. 

Climbing  was  a  leading  interest  Once  in  the  fifty-eighth  week, 
in  climbing  downstairs,  she  started  face  forward,  sitting  down  on 
a  step  and  putting  her  feet  over,  then  rising  and  standing  on  the 
lower  step,  sitting  down  and  putting  her  feet  over  again,  etc.  This, 
I  think,  was  the  method  she  used  in  getting  down  the  doorsteps, 
which  she  did  easily  and  safely;  but  it  was  evidently  too  tedious  for 
a  long  flight  of  steps,  and  after  thus  descending  two  or  three,  she 
gave  it  up,  and  turned  around  and  slid  down  swiftly  and  securely 
backward,  as  usual.  A  few  days  later  in  the  same  week,  and  again 
in  the  next,  I  note  her  liking  for  stepping  down  like  a  grown  person, 


364  University  of  California.  [Vol.  t. 

holding  some  one's  hand.  It  was  evident  she  did  not  like  the 
sliding  down  backward,  but  found  no  other  way  so  quick  and  safe. 
In  the  sixtieth  week  she  tried  hitching  from  step  to  step,  and  went 
down  the  whole  flight  thus,  without  any  help,  and  with  ease  and 
security.  This  saved  the  inconvenience  of  rising  and  sitting  down 
again  on  every  step,  but  cost  her  a  good  deal  of  jolting,  and  was 
not  kept  up  long.  In  the  fifty-ninth  week  I  found  she  could 
climb  the  stairs  in  the  dark  as  well  as  in  the  light. 

In  the  fifty-eighth  week  a  chest  that  usually  guarded  a  low 
window  (almost  level  with  the  floor)  chanced  to  be  moved,  and  the 
child  tried  to  get  out  of  the  window  (some  two  feet  from  the 
ground),  and  fell  out.  This  made  her  cautious,  but  for  a  day  or 
two  she  hovered  about  it,  and  made  essays  at  climbing  out,  but  did 
not  quite  dare  trust  herself  to  do  so.  Another  day  in  the  same 
week  she  amused  herself  by  climbing  up  on  a  footstool  (holding  to 
a  chair)  and  standing  on  it.  In  the  sixtieth  week  she  could  get  off 
a  lounge  or  low  chair  with  ease,  and  was  ambitious  of  climbing  up 
on  them  as  well.  One  day  early  in  the  week  she  was  much  elated 
at  finding  that  she  could  mount  a  small  trunk,  a  foot  high ;  she 
climbed  up,  sat  down  on  top,  and  climbed  off,  many  times.  When 
I  came  home  she  was  proud  to  show  me  the  achievement,  and  sat 
throned  on  the  trunk  looking  at  me  with  a  most  elated  expression; 
she  wished  to  keep  up  the  ascending  and  descending  for  some  time, 
and  kept  pointing  back  to  the  trunk  after  she  had  left  it.  Later  in 
the  week  she  succeeded  in  climbing  on  a  lounge  (15  inches  high), 
by  chancing  to  try  a  place  where  the  cushion  was  depressed  with 
long  use;  she  made  many  efforts  at  other  places,  but  the  slight 
difference  in  height  was  sufficient  to  defeat  her. 

In  this  month,  and  later,  she  was  impatient  of  being  held  (see 
Muscular  Sensation,  p.  185).  As  she  was,  however,  increasingly 
interested  in  understanding  and  using  speech,  she  was  on  the 
whole  willing  to  sit  longer  in  our  laps,   while  thus  entertained.1 


■In  this  month  and  the  preceding,  a  marked  trait  was  the  dela> sometimes 

many  seconds  —  that  the  child  would  make  before  seizing  an  object  offered,  or  i 
responding  to  a  suggestion.     It  seemed  as  if  she  were  considering  the  matter, 
but  even  when  the  proposition  was  highly  agreeable,  and  the  assent  sudden 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  365 


Fifteenth  Month  to  End  of  Third  Half- Year. 

At  fourteen  months  old  the  child  was  in  a  rudimentary  way  in 
possession  of  the  principal  race  movements  of  locomotion  and 
equilibrium.  Her  running,  it  is  true,  was  no  more  than  a  clumsy 
trot,  to  which  her  steps  would  quicken  in  eagerness,  as  in  hurrying 
to  meet  me  at  the  door;  but  I  doubt  whether  at  any  time  within 
the  first  three  years  it  was  the  real  gait  of  the  adult  runner,  with 
the  spring  from  the  foot.  Certainly  it  was  not  until  she  was 
directly  instructed,  in  the  sixth  year,  that  she  learned  to  keep  the 
heel  lifted  in  running,  instead  of  bringing  down  the  sole  flat  at 
every  step.  Some  children  do  much  better  than  this:  a  little 
cousin  of  hers  ran  swiftly  and  correctly  in  the  second  year. 

It  is  worth  notice  that  unlike  Preyer's  child,  who  began  with 
running  and  afterward  acquired  the  more  difficult  art  of  balancing 
at  a  slow  pace,  my  niece  came  to  running  by  gradual  acceleration 
of  her  walking.  In  the  sixty-fourth  week  she  ran  (or  trotted)  a 
great  deal;    in  the  sixty-fifth  walking  was  to  a  great  extent  aban- 


and  eager  when  it  did  come,  there  was  often  the  same  long,  apparently  con- 
templative pause  first.  I  attributed  it  to  the  time  required  for  cerebration,  but 
why  it  occurred  in  some  cases  and  not  in  others,  or  why  it  occurred  now  in 
cases  where  earlier  there  had  been  comparatively  prompt  seizing,  is  not  clear. 
It  is  possible  that  variations  in  attention  may  have  accounted  for  these  differ- 
ences,—  the  brain  may  have  been  somewhat  prepossessed  with  reverie,  even 
when  the  child  seemed  attentive;  a  condition  more  likely  now  than  earlier.  Or 
the  whole  action  of  taking  an  offered  object  may  have  risen  to  a  higher  psychic 
level,  involving  more  of  conscious  choice.  Where  the  use  of  words  was 
involved,  there  is  no  difficulty  in  supposing  the  necessary  cerebration  to  have 
caused  the  delay.  Certain  acts,  firmly  associated  by  practice  with  certain 
words,  as  when  the  child  was  told  to  pat  her  hands  or  to  do  some  other 
familiar  trick,  were  promptly  performed.  In  a  case  where  the  taught  associa- 
tion was  a  little  more  complex,  — that  of  waving  her  hand  and  saying  "  By-by" 
at  the  departure  of  any  person,  —though  the  action  was  usually  performed,  the 
person  addressed  would  sometimes  be  quite  out  of  sight  before  the  child  began 
her  adieus. 

In  general,  in  the  first  and  second  years,  all  reaction  times  were  slow,  but 
the  rule  had  exceptions,  the  law  of  which  I  did  not  detect:  certain  acts  were 
always  likely  to  be  more  quickly  performed.  (See  Grasping,  pp.  309,  316, 
321,  322.) 


366  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

doned,  and  she  scampered  about  habitually  at  a  jogging  run,  often 
upsetting  (either  by  tripping  or  by  overbalancing)  and  not  minding 
it  in  the  least.  Her  balance  in  walking  was  excellent  at  this  time: 
she  could  walk  impeded  with  heavy  or  clumsy  objects,  and  once  in 
the  sixty-second  week  essayed  to  bring  me  a  gown  that  she  wished 
me  to  put  on,  dragging  it  across  the  floor,  but  gave  it  up  and 
brought  me  a  pair  of  shoes  as  a  substitute;  she  could  lift  and  carry 
her  little  rocking-chair  (sixty-fourth  week);  and  in  the  sixty-fifth 
week  (fifteen  months  old),  when  frolicking  naked  by  the  fire,  if  told 
to  "kick"  or  "jump  high,"  would  kick  one  leg  out  quite  freely, 
without  losing  her  balance. 

In  the  sixteenth  month,  though  on  the  whole  firm  on  her  feet, 
and  constantly  running  about,  she  still  tripped  easily,  or  miscalcu- 
lated in  turning,  reaching,  etc.,  and  fell  down  a  good  many  times 
every  day.  She  was  easily  tilted  over,  —  e.  g.,  by  throwing  back 
her  head  to  look  upward.  Yet  she  showed  adaptiveness  often 
in  keeping  her  feet:  in  the  sixty-seventh  week,  after  walking  a 
few  moments  among  dead  leaves,  she  began  to  lift  her  feet  very 
high;  in  the  sixty-ninth,  she  stood  firm  in  a  tilting  rocking-chair, 
amused  herself  by  walking  back  and  forth  over  a  box  two  or  three 
inches  high,  and  stepped  easily  up  and  down  over  the  edge  of  the 
paved  veranda,  about  three  inches  high.  In  the  seventy-seventh 
week  she  walked  over  plowed  ground  pretty  well,  but  noticed  the 
inequalities,  saying,  "Down!"  as  she  descended  a  furrow,  noticing 
the  downward  inclination  once  or  twice  before  I  had  perceived 
it  myself. 

Her  joy  in  running  about,  her  passion  for  chasing  the  cats,  her 
ecstasy  over  playing  chase  around  the  furniture  (see  Muscular 
Sensation,  pp.  193-4),  her  running  away  from  us  with  laughter 
when  called,  are  noted  during  all  the  rest  of  the  half-year.  She 
was  happy  outdoors  hour  after  hour,  running  a  little  way,  stopping 
to  examine  things,  then  running  again.  One  day  in  the  sixty-sixth 
week  (sixteenth  month),  when  I  was  out  with  her  for  some  two 
hours,  she  was  running  and  walking  all  the  time,  with  about  four 
intervals  of  ten  minutes  each,  when  she  sat  digging,  etc.,  and  many 
intervals  of  a  few  seconds  to  examine  some  object.  I  thought  her 
tired  at  the  end  of  the  time,  but  she  was  unwilling  to  stop.  In  the 
seventy-first  week  (seventeenth  month)  seeing  the  door  ajar,  she 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  2>&7 

ran  away,  and  when  sought  in  a  few  seconds,  had  run  some  40  or 
50  yards  off  in  the  garden,  and  was  still  racing  off  as  hard  as  she 
could  go,  with  laughter  and  shouts  of  joy  in  her  liberty.  She  was 
enchanted  to  be  allowed  to  stand  naked  by  the  fire  and  caper  (see 
Muscular  Sensation,  p.  183),  but  in  the  seventy-first  week  tried 
perpetually  to  escape  and  scamper  about  the  room.  She  was  very 
quick  in  running  up  and  snatching  at  some  article  she  was  afraid 
she  would  not  get  otherwise  (seventy-first  week).  .  In  the  seventy- 
fourth  week,  she  would  cry  when  brought  indoors,  and  beg 
tragically,  "Go!  go!  go!"  When  outdoors  in  this  week,  she 
would  run  along  the  drive,  saying,  "Walk!  walk!  walk!"  in  high 
self-gratulation. 

Climbing  was  a  perpetual  desire.  I  have  mentioned  that  in  the 
fourteenth  month  the  child  tried  to  climb  into  chairs;  and  early  in 
the  fifteenth  she  set  herself  earnestly  to  accomplish  this.  In  the 
sixty-second  week  she  would  put  one  foot  on  the  rung  of  a  chair, 
and  holding  to  the  seat  would  lift  the  other  foot  from  the  floor; 
but  it  was  only  after  several  days'  effort  that  she  could  get  both 
feet  on  the  rung.  After  this  she  tried  persistently  to  pull  herself 
to  the  seat;  but  her  arms  were  too  short  to  get  hold  of  the  chair- 
back,  and  in  laying  hold  of  the  edges  of  the  seat  she  could  not  get 
a  straight  pull  and  so  lift  her  body.  She  got  her  knee  into  the  seat 
repeatedly,  but  could  get  no  higher.  Once  in  the  sixty-fourth 
week  some  one  gave  her  the  help  of  a  flat  hand  behind  to  brace 
against,  and  with  this  help  she  got  into  the  chair;  but  it  was  not 
till  after  about  nine  weeks  of  persistent  effort  that  she  finally 
climbed  into  a  chair  unaided  (see  next  page).  Meanwhile  she 
made  many  other  efforts  at  climbing.  She  tried  to  get  out  of  her 
baby-carriage,  saying,  "Down!  down!"  (sixty-second  week);  she 
climbed  over  a  pillow  to  the  lounge  arm,  and  stood  on  it  boldly; 
she  climbed  in  and  out  of  her  small  tin  bath-tub  (sixty-sixth  week); 
she  took  much  pleasure  in  climbing  up  and  standing  with  one  foot 
on  the  rung  of  one  chair,  the  other  on  the  rung  of  another  (sixty- 
seventh  week);  she  climbed  from  chairs  to  the  table  (sixty-eighth 
week).  In  this  week  she  could  scramble  down  unhelped  from  a 
bed,  sliding  backward  and  letting  herself  drop,  as  she  had  long 
done  in  getting  down  from  the  lounge.     She  was  at  first  afraid  to 


36S  University  of  California.  [Vol.  r. 

slide  down  from  one  bed,  which  was  somewhat  higher,  without  a 
slight  reassuring  touch  of  some  one's  hands  on  her;  but  by  the 
last  day  of  the  week  slid  down  alone  from  this  bed  also;  and  after 
this  it  soon  became  a  matter  of  course  to  slide  off  any  bed.  In  the 
sixty-ninth  week,  she  would  invert  a  strong  little  wooden  pail  she 
owned,  and  holding  to  her  mother,  would  climb  up  and  stand  on  it. 
(As  I  have  mentioned,  she  would  step  up  on  and  over  a  box  of  two 
or  three  inches  height;  but  for  any  height  much  over  this,  a  six- 
inch  stair,  for  instance,  she  had  to  climb,  pulling  up  with  her  arms, 
or  at  least  to  have  the  help  of  a  hand.)  She  climbed  a  good  deal 
over  the  arms  of  chairs,  etc.  She  was  not  altogether  secure  in 
these  ventures,  —  perhaps  a  little  incautious:  once  she  pitched 
backward  head  first  from  the  lounge,  where  she  was  moving  about; 
but  while  allowed  her  independence  in  climbing  she  was  always 
closely  watched,  so  she  only  pitched  into  some  one's  arms. 

In  the  seventeenth  month,  besides  the  invariable  pleasure  in 
free  activity  outdoors,  climbing  was  the  child's  greatest  interest. 
In  this  month  (seventy-first  week),  she  at  last  accomplished  her 
desire  of  months,  and  climbed  into  a  chair,  —  first  into  a  low 
steamer  chair,  and  then,  just  at  the  close  of  the  week,  into  a 
rather  low  arm-chair  (sixteen  inches)  whose  arms  were  easy  to  lay 
hold  upon;  the  cushion  chanced  to  be  out  of  it,  which  made  the 
ascent  easier.  On  gaining  the  seat,  the  child  exulted,  and  stood 
up  and  sat  down  with  great  satisfaction;  then  said,  "Down  !"  and 
let  herself  down  backward  in  her  usual  way.  She  evidently  felt 
that  she  was  very  high  up, —  perhaps  judging  her  height  by  the 
difficulty  of  the  ascent, —  for  though  she  had  repeatedly  slid  down 
from  beds  that  were  higher,  she  was  timid  this  time  about  letting 
go,  when  her  feet  were  but  an  inch  from  the  invisible  floor,  and 
stretched  herself  till  the  tip  of  a  toe  touched  before  she  would  let 
go.  She  immediately  climbed  back,  four  times  in  succession,  and 
later  returned  to  it  again;  she  showed  no  caution  in  sliding  out 
after  the  first  time.  Once,  while  standing  in  the  chair,  she  leaned 
back  and  tilted  it  in  jolts  for  some  time.  After  this,  she  quickly 
mastered  the  armless  chairs  (seventeen  inches  in  height);  and  in 
three  days  was  climbing  in  and  out  of  them  all.  By  the  end  of 
the  seventeenth  month,  she  climbed  into  chairs  and  sat  down  as 
a  matter  of  course. 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a  Child.  3^9 

In  the  seventy-second  week  she  experimented  at  stepping 
downstairs,  face  forward,  without  help,  almost  going  over  on  her 
nose  at  each  step;  she  would  rush  down  two  or  three  steps  in  this 
manner,  bringing  up  in  her  mother's  lap.  After  some  experiment- 
ing, she  preferred  again  to  steady  herself  on  the  stair  by  some  one's 
hand.  In  the  seventy-fourth  week  she  again  insisted  on  stepping 
down  without  help,  pushing  away  the  offered  hand ;  sometimes 
she  would  reach  up  a  hand  to  touch  banister  or  stair-rail,  some- 
times not.  She  now  stepped  down  very  well  and  steadily,  and  liked 
it;  but  one  of  us  always  kept  on  the  stair  just  below  her,  ready  to 
catch  her  in  case  of  a  stumble.  She  had  been,  all  these  latter 
weeks  of  the  seventeenth  month,  growing  very  bold,  and  barely 
escaped  bad  falls,  —  sometimes  did  not  escape  them.  In  the 
seventy-second  week  she  insisted  on  sitting  alone  on  the  piano 
stool,  and  pushed  away  my  hand;  yet  sat  so  insecurely  without 
it  that  I  always  held  to  her  dress.  In  the  seventy-third  week  she 
tried  to  climb  out  of  her  crib,  and  sought  new  places  to  climb; 
she  was  bewitched  with  climbing. 

In  the  eighteenth  month  she  scampered  about  the  house  when 
in  high  spirits,  climbing  from  chair  to  chair  with  inarticulate  sounds 
of  satisfaction.  In  the  seventy-seventh  week,  however,  she  showed 
a  certain  caution  about  falling  on  the  stairs,  heightened  doubtless 
by  warnings.  She  went  up  on  her  feet,  holding  by  the  banisters; 
but  coming  down  seemed  more  timid,  and  wished  me  close  to  her. 
She  recognized  a  place  where  she  had  fallen  a  few  days  before, 
and  turned  to  look  up  at  me,  saying,  "Fall!"  and  also  "Fall?" 
(There  is  where  I  fell.  Shall  I  fall  ?)  She  would  take  my  hand, 
then  push  it  away,  and  from  time  to  time  would  take  hold  of  my 
dress  for  an  instant  and  let  go  again.  She  would  start  down  a  flight 
face  forward,  hesitate,  observe,  "Fall!"  and  turn  around  and  begin 
to  scrabble  down  with  an  ejaculation  expressive  of  confidence  and 
satisfaction;  but  after  about  three  steps  she  would  stop,  get  to  her 
feet,  shake  herself  down  (taking  her  little  skirts  in  both  hands  and 
shaking  them,  with  evident  annoyance  in  the  way  they  got  huddled 
under  her),  and  start  again  to  step  down,  face  forward;  but  again 
showing  timidity,  yet  unwilling  to  let  me  give  a  hand,  would 
hesitate,  and  take  to  the  old  scrabbling  backward.  This  inter- 
change of  methods  happened  several  times  in  the  course  of  the 


37°  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

three  flights  from  garret  to  lower  floor.  While  stepping  down, 
she  was  annoyed  by  my  dress,  trailing  on  the  same  step  with  her, 
as  I  kept  a  step  below,  ready  to  catch  her  in  case  of  a  stumble; 
she  stopped  and  complained,  pointing  to  it,  "Dress!  'Way!"  As 
her  system  of  pronunciation  was  not  very  intelligible,  I  did  not  at 
first  catch  her  meaning,  and  she  repeated  the  protest  several  times. 
The  next  day  she  was  still  more  timid,  and  even  clung  to  me,  on 
the  lower  stair,  where  there  are  no  banisters  to  hold  to,  the  stair 
going  between  walls;  on  the  upper  flights,  where  she  could  put  a 
hand  on  the  banisters,  she  was  proud  to  call  my  attention  to  the 
fact  that  she  was  walking  like  a  lady:  "Lady!  Ruth!"  Later 
in  the  week,  starting  boldly  downstairs,  she  would  stop,  waver, 
say  "Fall!"  and  turn  round  and  back  down. 

I  set  her  on  a  carpenter's  horse,  perhaps  six  feet  high,  one  day 
in  this  week:  she  was  rather  sober,  but  willing,  and  did  not  care 
to  cling  to  me,  satisfied  as  long  as  I  held  her  dress  firmly;  but 
when  I  would  have  set  her  on  her  feet  there,  she  objected,  and 
seemed  timid.  The  same  day,  however,  she  was  desirous  of 
climbing  a  ladder,  and  pleased  when  I  helped  her.  A  few  days 
later,  she  was  anxious  to  reach  books  on  a  higher  shelf  than  she 
had  before  reached;  standing  in  a  chair,  she  soon  reached  and 
strained  to  get  higher;  I  gave  her  two  dictionaries  to  stand  on, 
and  she  climbed  them  with  little  fear,  and  then  wanted  to  climb 
up  on  the  shelves.  Yet  not  a  week  before,  she  had  fallen  back- 
ward from  a  chair  while  reaching  after  books,  and  had  been  a 
good  deal  frightened. 

In  the  seventy-eighth  week,  she  climbed  into  her  high-chair. 
She  did  it  with  difficulty,  and  failed  when  she  began  with  the  left 
foot.  She  then  set  her  right  foot  very  high  up,  on  the  foot-rest 
of  the  chair,  and  pulled  up  with  sheer  strength  of  arms  till  she 
got  her  left  knee  on  the  seat,  then  her  right,  then  turned  around 
and  sat  down  easily.  Her  mother  held  the  chair  still  while  she 
did  this. 

At  some  time  between  the  fifty-eighth  week  (when  I  noted 
definitely  that  she  could  not  do  it)  and  the  sixty-fifth,  she  had 
learned  to  sit  down  backward  in  her  little  rocking-chair,  or  on  a 
stool.      In  the  sixty-fifth   and  sixty-sixth  weeks  she  took   much 


SHINN.J 


The  Development  cf  a   Child.  371 


pride  in  seating  herself  on  her  wooden  pail:  she  would  pick  it  up, 
invert  it  carefully,  turn  her  back,  and  cautiously  sit  down.  Often 
she  missed  it,  but  did  not  fall,  as  she  was  on  her  guard  and  lowered 
herself  with  tense  muscles;  she  would  try  again  until  she  succeeded 
in  aiming  right.  One  day  in  the  sixty-sixth  week  she  took  her  tin 
plate  from  the  table  and  carried  it  about  the  room,  inverting  it  and 
sitting  down  on  it.  Throughout  the  sixteenth  month  sitting  down 
backward  continued  to  interest  her  more  or  less,  and  once  in  the 
seventeenth  month  (seventieth  week),  seeing  a  tin  wash-basin  within 
reach,  she  took  it,  set  it  on  the  floor,  and  tried  to  sit  down  in  it 
missed  it  once,  but  tried  again  and  succeeded.  This  interested  her 
much  and  she  sat  in  it  for  some  time,  rocking  a  little.  It  was  her 
favorite  amusement  all  day  to  carry  this  basin  around  and  sit  down 
in  it.  By  the  beginning  of  the  eighteenth  month  she  could  sit 
down  backward  easily  in  her  little  chair,  and  rock  it  nicely  and 
smoothly,  —  this  last  a  new  accomplishment  (seventy-fifth  week), 
and  much  enjoyed. 

During  all  this  period  a  noticeable  trait  was  the  child's  interest 
in  doing  something  novel  with  her  body,  apparently  in  mere 
curiosity  and  sense  of  power.  The  experiments  in  sitting  down  on 
the  bucket  and  in  the  basin  are  instances,  and  there  were  many 
others,  from  the  fifteenth  month  on.  In  the  sixty-first  week  she 
found  that  she  could  walk  about  with  her  head  thrown  back, 
looking  at  the  ceiling,  and  practiced  it  for  a  long  time.  In  the 
sixty-fifth  week  she  walked  about  in  her  father's  slippers,  lifting  her 
encumbered  feet  skilfully.  At  sixteen  months  old  she  hit  upon  the 
feat  of  walking  backward,  and  practiced  it  all  day,  much  interested 
and  amused.  The  next  day  some  one  showed  her  how  to  put  her 
hands  behind  her;  this  interested  her  much,  and  she  would  do  it 
whenever  asked.  Seeing  her  pleasure  in  any  new  use  of  her  body, 
I  showed  her  how  to  fold  her  arms;  but  the  little  arms  were  too 
fat,  and  she  could  only  cross  them,  which  she  did  all  day  with 
much  satisfaction. 

In  the  seventieth  week  her  great  joy  when  indoors  was  a  shallow 
box,  some  zyi  inches  high,  on  which  she  would  stand,  stepping  on 
and  off  with  endless  pleasure.  Once  she  fell,  and  sat  down  hard  on 
the  edge,  but  usually  had  no  trouble  in  stepping  on  and  off.     In  the 


37 2  University  of  California.  [Vol.  j. 

seventy-second  week  she  saw  two  shallow  boxes,  one  set  on  the 
other,  and  tried  to  climb  up  on  them;  to  prevent  this  unsafe 
performance,  her  grandmother  placed  them  both  on  the  floor,  some 
three  inches  apart;  the  child  then  began  to  get  up  on  one  and  step 
over  to  the  other,  with  much  pride.  She  kept  this  play  up  for  some 
time;  and  later,  after  amusing  herself  awhile  in  another  room,  she 
suddenly  abandoned  her  employments  and  hastened  back  into  the 
room  where  the  cherry  boxes  had  been,  looked  around  till  she 
spied  them  set  up  sidewise  against  the  wall,  and  immediately  began 
to  pull  them  out,  saying,  "  Box!"  Getting  one,  she  started  to  carry 
it  to  where  they  had  stood  before.  Her  mother  put  the  two  boxes 
in  a  convenient  place,  and  there  the  child  stepped  up  on  one,  over 
to  the  other,  and  back  and  forth  and  down  for  some  time,  very  hap- 
pily; then  picked  up  one  and  carried  it  —  an  armful  —  out  into  the 
other  room,  where  she  tried  placing  it  in  different  spots  and  stand- 
ing on  it,  then  in  another  room,  and  at  last  took  it  back  to  the  side 
of  the  other  box,  and  tried  to  set  it  down  there,  getting  it  partly 
on  top  of  the  other;  but  finally  set  it  in  its  former  place. 

It  was  in  the  last  week  of  the  seventeenth  month  that  I  tried 
the  experiment  of  letting  her  hang  by  her  hands,  and  found  her 
well  able  to  do  it,  and  on  the  whole  pleased  with  the  feeling 
(Muscular  Sensation,  p.  192).  Creeping  was  now  unfamiliar 
enough  to  be  a  novelty;  and  in  the  eighteenth  month  (seventy- 
fifth  week)  she  was  charmed  when  her  mother  pursued  her  on 
hands  and  knees  across  the  carpet  and  begged  her  to  continue. 
The  same  day,  having  stumbled  and  fallen,  she  amused  herself  by 
running  around,  flinging  herself  down,  and  crying,  "  Down!"  Now 
and  then  she  would  turn  round  and  round,  saying,  "Round!  round!" 
("Wow!  wow!")  She  was  happy  many  minutes  in  the  seventy-sixth 
week  stepping  from  one  chair  to  another,  a  more  adventurous 
repetition  of  her  play  with  the  boxes  the  month  before.  Just  at  the 
end  of  this  week  she  was  greatly  pleased  with  the  discovery  that 
when  she  had  seated  herself  on  her  inverted  bucket  she  could  tip  it 
forward,  bringing  herself  to  her  knees. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  Zl Z 

Fourth  Half- Year. 

At  a  year  and  a  half  old  the  child  was  already  so  far  in  posses- 
sion of  her  powers  of  locomotion  as  to  have  almost  all  the  pleasure 
in  their  exercise  that  can  be  had  at  any  time  of  life,  while  their 
novelty  still  made  their  ordinary  daily  use  a  keen  happiness. 
During  the  next  half-year  my  notes  record  an  intense  and  un- 
changing delight  in  free  movement  outdoors.  The  child  had  almost 
never  asked  to  be  taken  since  getting  the  use  of  her  own  legs,  and 
would  rarely  consent  to  sit  still  in  a  lap.  As  before,  she  chased  the 
cats  (ninetieth  week);  ran  away  laughing  to  be  pursued  (dining  the 
whole  half-year);  and  delighted,  with  ecstasies  of  laughter,  in  playing 
chase  around  the  table  (nineteenth  and  twentieth  months  especially; 
see  p.  194),  and  in  romping  of  all  sorts.  She  ran  about  after  butter- 
flies (eighty-first  week),  and  frolicked  with  the  dog  (eighty-seventh 
week,  ninetieth  week,  see  p.  199);  she  was  fond  of  stepping  from 
board  to  board  of  an  old  cider-mill  frame  that  lay  on  the  ground 
(eighty-seventh  week,  ninetieth  week).  Sometimes,  especially  in  the 
later  months,  she  would  find  less  active  occupations,  as  digging  in 
the  ground,  or  picking  flowers;  but  sometimes  she  would  trot 
about  from  one  thing  to  another,  without  interesting  herself  long 
in  anything,  or  would  run  to  and  fro,  rejoicing  in  the  mere  power 
of  balanced  motion,  and  exulting  aloud,  —  "Ou'  doo'!  Ou'  doo'!" 
(eighty-fourth  week),  or  in  the  eighty-sixth  week,  "Walk!  'On  two 
little  feet ' !  "  (quoting  a  jingle),  —  or  going  into  ecstasies  of  freedom 
and  activity,  capering  and  ejaculating  (see  pp.  239,  247,    25  1). 

Her  perpetual  appeal  in  the  twentieth  month  was,  "Walk  out- 
doors!" If  she  was  carried,  or  any  one  tried  to  make  her  go  in  an 
undesired  direction,  she  would  repeat  it  earnestly,  —  walk  appar- 
ently meaning  walk  freely.  Once  in  this  month  she  was  taken  on  a 
trip  of  two  days,  including  a  number  of  changes  of  cars,  and  a  stop 
for  errands  in  the  city;  at  all  intervals  she  wished  to  be  walking  or 
running:  at  the  station,  she  walked  up  and  down  a  bench,  saying, 
"Back  go  Ruth!  "at  each  turn;  at  a  restaurant,  and  at  a  friend's 
office,  she  was  excited  and  eager,  slipping  down  from  her  chair  and 
wishing  to  run  off  and  explore  everything;  on  the  street,  she  was 
determined  to  go  on  her  own  feet,  reluctant  even  to  be  led,  running 
ahead  along  the  sidewalk  with  her  curious  rolling  gait,  laughing 


374  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

with  joy,  and  only  occasionally  stopping  to  look  at  anything;  if  we 
stopped  for  shopping,  she  was  soon  bored  in  the  store,  in  spite  of  all 
its  sights,  and  was  begging  to  "go  ou'  doo',  —  wa'  [walk]  ou'  doo'!  " 
She  ran  thus  for  some  ten  blocks  in  all,  carried  over  crossings. 
When  we  reached  the  village  to  which  we  were  bound,  she  was 
most  discontented  at  our  getting  into  a  carriage,  and  begged  to  get 
out  and  walk,  and  as  we  found  that  the  carriage  did  not  start  for 
some  minutes,  we  indulged  her;  when  the  carriage  overtook  us, 
she  was  still  reluctant  to  get  in,  begging,  "Walk!  walk!"  and  on 
reaching  the  house  we  were  to  visit,  she  would  pay  attention  to  no 
one,  clinging  to  her  mother  and  begging,  "  Ou'  doo'! — wa'  ou' 
doo'!"  The  next  morning  she  insisted  on  running  about  on  her 
own  feet  in  the  strange  places  for  nearly  four  hours,  with  the 
greatest  enjoyment,  —  traversing  perhaps  a  mile  in  all.  In  passing 
through  the  city  again  on  our  return  the  same  day,  she  ran  as 
before  along  the  sidewalk,  waving  her  arms  and  shouting  with  joy 
of  liberty  (a  somewhat  conspicuous  little  object,  which  made  many 
passers  turn  and  smile,  —  a  contrast  to  the  other,  demurer  babies 
on  the  street);  and  on  the  ferry-boat  she  insisted  on  trotting  round 
and  round  the  deck,  and  was  reluctant  to  stop  and  go  ashore. 

After  this,  she  preferred  walking  even  to  driving,  and  one  day 
at  the  end  of  the  month,  hearing  something  said  about  driving  as 
her  bonnet  was  put  on,  was  disposed  to  whimper:  "Walk,  —  walk 
outdoors!"  she  protested.  At  the  end  of  the  twenty-first  month 
again,  she  went  off  to  drive  protesting  that  she  wished  to  "walk 
on  feet."  Returning  at  about  5:30  in  the  afternoon,  after  being 
outdoors  since  breakfast,  her  first  greeting  to  me  was,  "Aunty! 
Ruth  walk  outdoors!"  (1.  e.,  wishes  to).  She  protested  bitterly 
against  going  into  the  house  to  receive  a  toy  wagon  which  had 
been  brought  for  her  in  her  absence,  wriggled  away  from  it, 
and  consented  to  take  pleasure  in  it  only  when  it  was  brought 
outdoors. 

She  had  no  timidity  about  wandering  off  alone;  and  we  often 
allowed  her  to  do  it,  watching  at  a  little  distance.  Possibly  she 
experienced  an  added  sense  of  freedom  and  power  from  the  idea 
that  she  was  alone.  In  the  ninetieth  week  she  started  off  on  a 
walk,  and  we  followed.  She  tramped  out  along  the  drive  to 
the  main  road,  and  tried  to  climb  a  fence  to  50  to  a  neighbor's 


shinh.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  375 

and  see  his  ducks  and  geese;  failing  in  this,  and  also  in  an 
attempt  to  creep  under  the  fence,  she  kept  on  along  the  high- 
way, running  ahead  by  herself,  rushing  at  the  dog  and  mauling 
him,  or  standing  and  laughing  as  he  charged  past  her.  After 
she  had  gone  about  360  yards  she  stopped  and  tried  again  to 
climb  the  fence,  and  after  failing  in  this  and  being  stopped  in 
persistent  attempts  to  explore  a  neighbor's  lane,  turned  home- 
ward, and  walked  some  18  yards  more,  till  her  father,  coming  by  in 
a  buggy,  took  her  up.  About  this  time  she  began  to  take  wider 
ranges  in  her  travels  round  the  garden,  playing  a  little  while  on 
the  walks,  then  roaming  off  into  the  regions  of  potatoes  and  corn 
beyond. 

On  a  trip  to  the  city  in  the  ninetieth  week,  she  behaved  just 
as  she  had  done  the  month  before,  —  insisting  on  being  on  her 
own  feet  in  the  street,  and  running  ahead  at  the  top  of  her  speed, 
unconcerned  if  she  got  far  ahead;  indifferent  to  the  attractions  of 
the  shops,  and  eager  to  leave  them  and  "walk."  At  one  time, 
for  a  number  of  blocks,  she  persisted  in  sitting  down  on  every 
step  and  curb  she  passed;  when  carried  to  prevent  this,  she 
struggled,  and  cried,  "Let  Ruth  walk!"  On  a  street-car  she 
insisted  on  walking  back  and  forth  along  the  seat  that  ran  along 
one  side  (empty,  as  it  chanced),  enjoying  the  way  she  was  flung 
about  by  the  jolts  of  the  car.  In  all  such  cases,  as  long  as  she 
was  doing  no  harm  and  annoying  no  one,  we  let  her  take  her 
own  way,  keeping  close  enough  to  her  in  safe  places  for  a  vigilant 
watch,  and  in  unsafe  ones,  close  enough  for  instant  interference; 
but  allowing  her  to  feel  as  unrestricted  as  possible. 

Camping  in  the  redwood  forest  in  the  twenty-second  month, 
she  objected  at  first  to  the  rough  ground,  and  wished  to  be  car- 
ried. But  on  the  fourth  day  a  stray  dog  roamed  into  camp, 
and  in  her  interest  in  him  she  ran  after  him  over  stones  and 
sand  in  the  stream-bed,  and  tried  to  scramble  up  the  steep  bank 
after  him  when  he  went  away.  After  this  she  was  more  inde- 
pendent, and  disposed  to  walk  around  by  herself.  On  her  return 
from  this  trip  she  was  more  eager  than  ever  to  stay  outdoors  and 
ramble  round:  developed  some  crossness  under  restraint,  unusual 
to  her  even  disposition,  and  "Ruth  go  outdoors!"  "Ruth  stay 
outdoors!"  was  her  constant  cry.     "Let  Ruth  down!"  she  cried, 


2>7&  University  of  California.  vol.  i. 

held  in  my  arms  merely  for  the  moment  that  I  lifted  her  from 
the  wagon  on  her  return  from  a  drive:  and  set  on  the  ground, 
she  trotted  about,  pointing  down  every  path  and  saying,  "That 
where  Ruth  goes!"  a  curious  expression  of  conscious,  remembered 
joy  in  her  pilgrimages. 

Indoors  also  she  often  raced  about  in  a  random  way,  in  sheer 
exuberance  of  spirits  and  activity,  in  only  less  glee  than  when  out- 
doors, capering,  uttering  joyous  cries  and  prattle,  impatient  if  held 
still  a  moment, —  "O  aunty,  let  Tootyboo  get  down!"  "O  papa, 
let  Tootyboo  walk ! " 

I  have  before  mentioned  especially  her  remarkable  spirits  and 
desire  of  motion  in  the  twenty-first  month,  her  running  about  all 
day,  shouting  and  squealing,  and  crying,  "I-ya!  i-ya!";  her  ecstasy 
in  scampering  about  naked,  flinging  herself  down  and  jumping  up, 
etc.  (eighty-ninth  week,  ninety-fifth  week;  see  p.  183.) 

She  still  tripped  and  fell  often  enough  to  show  that  her  balance 
was  not  really  firm  yet.  She  probably  went  down  with  relaxed 
muscles,  and  seemed  to  feel  very  little  inconvenience  from  the 
tumbles,  and  to  regard  them  as  matters  of  course.  "  Down  you 
went!"  I  commented  once  as  she  fell.  "Go  down  more,"  answered 
the  child  serenely,  tripping  again  as  she  picked  herself  up  and  went 
on.  She  often  commented  on  a  fall:  "Fell  down  that  time!" 
(ninetieth  week);  "Take  care!"  when  she  slipped  or  stumbled, 
(ninety-first  week).  Once  she  missed  her  chair  in  sitting  down 
backward,  and  sat  in  a  heap  on  the  floor;  she  picked  herself  up 
laughing,  not  chagrined,  but  made  self-conscious.  I  have  but  one 
record  (at  twenty  months  old)  of  a  fall  that  frightened  or  discomfited 
her  at  all,  when  walking  or  running;  and  this  time  she  was  playing 
with  another  child,  and  was  perhaps  overset  by  some  movement  of 
hers.  Once  in  the  twenty-first  month  I  saw  her  stand  on  one  foot 
in  the  bath  to  scrub  the  other  knee  with  a  brush,  balancing  only 
with  a  slight  touch  of  one  hand  on  the  edge  of  the  tub. 

The  desire  to  climb  continued  remarkable,  and  the  half-year 
showed  a  good  deal  of  progress  in  skill  in  this  direction.  I  have 
above  related  (see  Muscular  Sensation)  that  in  the  eighty-third 
week  the  child  climbed  one  flight  of  stairs  ten  times  in  succession, 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Cliild.  T>77 

turning  around  and  going  back  as  soon  as  she  reached  the  bottom, 
and  also  took  two  or  three  trips  up  and  down  another  flight;  then 
after  a  brief  excursion  into  one  of  the  rooms,  ascended  the  stairs 
again, —  about  160  steps  in  all.  This  climbing  was  stepping  up 
from  stair  to  stair,  holding  by  the  banisters.  I  have  many  notes  of 
her  pleasure  in  the  exercise.  She  liked  especially  the  garret  stairs, 
which  were  guarded  with  banisters  in  an  open  well,  while  the  lower 
stairs  were  inclosed  between  walls  and  gave  her  nothing  to  hold 
to,  as  the  stair-rail  was  too  high  for  her.  On  this  lower  stair  she 
went  down  cautiously,  remarking,  "  Might  fall!"  but  on  the  upper 
one  she  was  proud  to  climb  without  having  me  close  at  hand.  She 
handed  articles  to  me  to  carry,  but  did  not  wish  to  be  helped 
beyond  this,  even  on  the  lower  stair:  at  eighty-eight  and  eighty- 
nine  weeks  old,  she  would  say,  "Own  self!  own  self!"  when  help 
was  offered,  or  "Ruth  go  downstairs  own  self!"  or,  "Let  Ruth 
walk!"  Once  in  the  eighty-ninth  week  she  slipped  away  and  went 
upstairs  quite  alone,  stepping,  not  creeping,  without  any  timidity. 
Climbing  the  stairs  once,  in  this  same  week,  to  avoid  setting  her 
foot  on  a  pasteboard  box  that  stood  on  one  step,  instead  of  bring- 
ing her  left  foot  to  her  right,  she  carried  it  past,  to  a  higher  step,  as 
an  adult  does,  and  as  she  had  formerly  done  when  walking  up  with 
the  help  of  a  hand;  and  with  the  help  of  the  banister,  she  made 
the  extra  step  with  little  difficulty.  Going  down  the  same  day,  she 
stepped  several  steps  quite  unaided,  without  touching  a  banister, 
but  usually  preferred  to  hold  lightly.  In  the  one  hundredth  week, 
I  was  told  that  she  had  started  upstairs  after  some  one,  and  had 
fallen,  —  the  first  real  fall  on  the  stairs  that  I  have  recorded,  though 
I  have  notes  of  her  stumbling,  —  and  cried  hard,  but  insisted  on 
going  up  again.  The  next  day,  when  reminded  of  it  and  asked  if 
it  hurt  her,  she  seemed  somewhat  annoyed  and  abashed,  and  said 
''No,  —  that  didn't  hurt  Ruth  anything!"  —  perhaps  fearing  her 
freedom  in  climbing  the  stairs  might  be  restricted. 

In  the  eighty-seventh  week  she  took  pleasure  in  sliding  down- 
stairs, sitting  on  a  step  and  sliding  gradually  over  the  edge,  coming 
down  with  a  thud,  saying  as  she  did  so,  "O,  o,  o,  o-o."  In  the 
ninety-first  she  liked  to  slide  down  on  her  feet,  holding  some  one's 
hand,  and  sliding  from  step  to  step  with  a  racking  jolt  (see  p.  204, 
205,  207,  note). 


3/S  University  of    California.  [Vol.  i. 

She  had  many  other  ambitions  in  climbing,  apart  from  the 
stairs.  She  wished,  as  earlier,  to  climb  up  on  tables  and  desks  and 
explore  pigeon-holes,  examine  pens,  etc.  (eightieth  week,  e.  g.). 
"Climb  up,  climb  up!"  she  said  (eighty-ninth  week),  pretending  to 
climb  a  tree;  in  the  ninetieth  week,  she  climbed  up  five  steps  of  a 
steep  ladder-stair,  saying,  "Ladder!"  She  climbed  down  with  the 
help  of  my  hands,  stepping  securely  enough,  and  turned  to  climb 
again,  but  gave  up  after  one  step,  apparently  a  little  intimidated  by 
the  difficulty  of  the  descent.  Two  days  later,  at  a  neighbor's, 
several  older  children  took  her  up  a  ladder  to  a  hay  loft:  she  went 
with  entire  confidence,  needing  no  help  in  climbing,  and  after  she 
had  been  brought  down,  went  up  half  a  dozen  steps  several  times, 
with  no  one  behind  her,  only  arms  below  ready  to  catch  her  at 
need.  Later  in  the  same  week  I  found  her  standing  on  a  board 
that  propped  the  clothes-line,  teetering  it  up  and  down  and  crying, 
"Ruth  ride!  "  Presently  she  sat  down  astride  it,  and  continued  to 
teeter.  At  my  suggestion,  she  took  my  hands  and  walked  up  the 
steep  incline  to  the  top;  and  on  reaching  the  ground  she  wished  to 
do  it  again. 

She  now  (ninety-first  week)  began  climbing  ladders  wherever 
she  saw  them,  going  halfway  up,  to  "pick  leaves,"  e.  g.,  — getting 
her  leaves,  and  coming  down  successfully.  I  always  allowed  her 
to  do  this,  standing  close  by  —  or  following  her  if  she  went  above 
my  reach  —  with  a  firm  hold  on  her  skirts,  or  with  my  arms  ready 
to  close  about  her  at  the  slightest  stumble.  She  climbed  very 
firmly  and  cautiously,  however,  and  I  have  no  record  or  memory 
of  ever  seeing  her  make  a  slip.  Climbing,  indeed,  where  hands 
and  feet  are  both  used,  seems  to  me  a  safer  exercise  for  a  baby  than 
people  think:  on  an  exposed  high  place,  standing  or  sitting,  still 
more  walking,  a  little  thing  is  apt  to  lose  balance,  but  when  climb- 
ing with  all  fours,  so  to  speak,  conscious  of  the  insecurity,  it  seems 
quite  able  to  take  care  of  itself. 

At  just  twenty-one  months  old,  the  child  climbed  a  ladder  to 
the  top,  all  but  the  very  top  ledge,  over  which  she  leaned  and 
looked  down  the  other  side  without  the  least  timidity,  —  nine 
steps.  She  started  up  another,  but  after  some  five  steps  came 
to  a  step  that  was  worn  narrow,  and  at  once  stopped,  said,  "  Go 
down ! "  and  climbed  back.    She  then  wished  to  be  put  up  into  a  seat 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  379 

on  the  high  judge's  stand,  above  the  tennis  court;  she  had  before 
been  timid  about  this  seat,  but  now  when  I  held  to  her  dress,  she 
pushed  away  my  hand  with  annoyance;  sat  awhile,  interested  in 
the  height,  then  scrambled  out  of  the  seat,  observing  that  she 
wished  to  sit  "down  there,"  on  the  platform  itself,  picked  her  way 
carefully  out  on  the  narrow  ledge  of  platform  beside  the  chair, 
then  started  to  let  herself  down  backward  (four  feet)  without 
fear.  In  a  hammock,  she  was  much  annoyed  by  my  keeping  hold 
of  the  edges,  but  came  near  falling  out  when  she  tried  to  push 
my  hands  away,  and  gave  up  her  resistance.  I  note  on  the  next 
day  that  she  is  at  this  period  "possessed  to  climb."  She  would 
even  try,  when  driving,  to  climb  up  the  rods  that  supported  the 
phaeton  top. 

During  the  fortnight  in  camp  in  the  twenty-second  month, 
she  was  at  first  timid  about  climbing,  as  she  was  about  walking 
on  the  rough  ground.  The  tent  was  at  the  top  of  a  bank  that 
descended  quite  steeply  to  the  creek  bed,  and  at  first  she  had  to 
be  carried  here:  but  after  the  fourth  day,  when  she  ran  after  a 
dog  (see  above)  and  tried  to  climb  the  bank  after  him,  she  lost 
timidity,  and  insisted  on  being  allowed  to  go  up  and  down  between 
tent  and  camp  fire  on  her  own  feet,  with  help,  and  once  or  twice 
started  alone,  sliding  recklessly  down  the  steep  incline.  She 
clambered  up  into  a  high  seat  her  father  had  made  her  between 
two  saplings,  and  amused  herself  by  sitting  on  the  arm  of  it. 
Her  mother  records  that  she  climbed  up  the  low-sloping  roots 
of  a  redwood,  to  where  they  joined  the  trunk,  and  taking  hold 
of  a  projecting  bit  of  bark,  set  her  little  foot  up  against  the  200- 
foot  column  of  the  tree,  and  made  a  tremendous  effort  to  ascend, 
saying,  "Ruth  climb  tree!"  She  tried  to  go  up  the  bank  alone 
but  had  to  give  it  up,  saying,  "Ruth  can't  go  up  this  hill!" 

In  the  ninety-fifth  week,  after  her  return,  she  saw  a  ladder  flat 
against  a  wall,  and  rushed  to  climb  it,  and  tugged  up  some  steps; 
then,  thinking  it  not  safe,  I  asked  her  uncle  to  set  it  out  from  the 
wall;  he  did  so,  but  left  it  very  steep,  perhaps  at  half  the  angle 
used  by  the  men  in  picking  fruit;  she  climbed  it,  however,  to  the 
top,  saying  over  and  over,  "See  this  child  climb  ladder!"  (which 
I  may  have  said  in  calling  her  uncle.)  Whenever  it  shook,  she 
said  to  me,  as  I  climbed  behind  her,  "This  ladder  won't  fall."      At 


380  University  of  California.  Vol.  1. 

the  top,  she  reached  up  higher,  disappointed  to  stop.  I  set  her 
on  the  top,  where  she  sat  fearlessly,  and  when  once  I  had  set  her 
foot  on  the  first  step  of  the  descent  again,  scrambled  down  with- 
out help.  I  held  her  dress  firmly  all  the  time,  without  giving  her 
any  aid  in  climbing.  She  wished  to  ascend  again  at  once,  and 
asked  to  "climb  that  ladder,"  pointing  to  the  house-wall  (where  the 
"rustic"  boarding  showed  projecting  edges),  and  up  to  the  wide 
ledge  over  a  door-frame. 

In  the  ninety-sixth  week,  she  was  much  displeased  if  she  was 
set  in  her  high-chair;  she  must  always  climb  up.  She  was  proud 
of  this  ability,  and  usually  called  to  me  to  "look  at  Ruth  climb!" 

From  the  fifteenth  month  the  child  had  in  a  way  understood 
the  word  jump,  and  would  try  to  obey  the  direction,  but  only  by- 
springing  with  her  body,  bending  her  knees,  but  not  lifting  her 
feet  from  the  ground.  This  springing  movement  with  the  trunk 
muscles  seemed  very  instinctive,  beginning,  as  I  have  said,  in  the 
earliest  months,  and  continuing  still  as  a  demonstration  of  joy  and 
excitement,  —  e.  g.,  when  she  was  camping  in  the  twenty-second 
month  and  I  joined  the  party,  on  seeing  me  at  the  car  window 
she  jumped  up  and  down  in  a  marked  manner,  though  she  was 
not  able  to  lift  her  feet  and  jump  clear.  She  was  ambitious  to 
do  so,  and  once  in  the  eighty-second  week  practised  a  long 
time,  trying  to  learn  the  movement  from  her  parents:  she  would 
watch  them,  and  bend  her  knees  just  as  they  did,  but  could  not 
raise  both  feet.  In  the  ninety-first  week,  a  few  days  before  she 
was  twenty-one  months  old,  seeing  an  older  child,  a  boy,  jumping 
along  the  paved  veranda,  she  ran  along  behind  him,  eager  to 
imitate,  squatting  down  and  making  a  motion  with  her  body  as 
if  to  jump,  then  running  forward  about  the  space  of  his  jump, 
and  squatting  again  when  he  did,  and  so  on.  This  was  only  one 
of  many  efforts  to  imitate  jumping.  She  did  not,  however,  seem 
to  be  aware  of  her  failure,  but  pranced  cheerfully,  and  called  it 
jumping.  One  day  in  the  ninety-seventh  week,  when  she  was 
making  many  experiments,  as  squatting  and  waddling  (see  below) 
she  tried  a  great  deal  to  jump  off  a  step,  squatting  as  if  to  jump, 
and  then  stepping  off,  —  yet  several  times  did  approach  nearly  to 
a  real  jump.     At  twenty-three  months  old,  she  caught  the  move- 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  3&  I 

ment,  and  began  really  to  jump  with  both  feet.  She  was  delighted 
with  her  achievement  of  this  long-desired  accomplishment,  and  for 
the  next  ten  days  I  have  frequent  notes  of  her  practising  it  (up  to 
the  one  hundred  and  second  week);  after  that,  it  ceased,  apparently, 
to  interest  so  much.  She  seems  to  have  struck  it  at  last  by  pure 
accident. 

As  in  the  preceding  months  she  liked  new  experiments  in 
using  her  body.  She  amused  herself  with  trying  to  walk  about 
with  eyes  shut  and  covered  with  her  hands,  and  kept  this  up  till 
she  had  had  several  falls  (eighty-fourth  week);  walking  on  the 
road,  she  enjoyed  scuffing  along  in  dusty  places  (eighty-sixth 
week);  she  continued  occasionally  to  experiment  at  walking  back- 
ward; she  squatted  down  and  in  this  position  waddled  along  (ninety- 
seventh  week),  much  amused  at  the  curious  gait,  crying,  "Ruth 
walk  this  way!"  She  amused  herself  a  long  time  running  about 
while  leaning  back  as  far  as  possible,  then  squatting  and  waddling. 
She  was  interested  also,  the  same  day,  in  efforts  to  jump,  (see 
above);  and  in  creeping  about  the  lawn,  saying,  "See  Ruth 
crawl!"  (I  find  an  occasional  reference  to  her  creeping  under  the 
table,  but  on  the  whole  the  quadrupedal  mode  of  progression 
seems  to  have  been  promptly  abandoned  as  soon  as  she  could 
walk.)  In  the  twenty-third  month,  she  was  enchanted  to  be  run 
off  her  feet  by  two  people  taking  each  an  arm  (holding  her  well 
supported  with  arm  and  hand)  and  running,  either  on  a  level  or 
downstairs.  She  would  run  to  the  other  members  of  the  family, 
crying,  "Ruth  ran!  Ruth  did  run  this  way!"  with  excited 
gestures;  her  own  intensified  activity,  rather  than  the  passive 
motion,  seemed  to  impress  her. 

Third   Year. 

In  the  third  year,  my  notes  record  little  farther  development  of 
the  movements  of  balance  and  locomotion,  —  which  were,  in  fact, 
practically  acquired  for  life  before  the  close  of  the  second  year. 
With  the  increasing  development  of  intelligence,  the  child's  interests 
and  occupations  grew  more  varied,  and  the  mere  pleasure  of  using 
her  bodily  powers  fell  into  the  background.  When  she  was  taken 
to  the  city  on  the  day  after  her  second  birthday  and  thereafter, 


382  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

instead  of  caring  only  to  run  on  the  sidewalk,  she  was  silently  and 
happily  occupied  on  the  cars  in  watching  the  people,  and  the  sights 
from  the  windows,  and  accompanied  us  decorously  on  the  streets 
and  in  the  shops.  Her  desire  to  be  outdoors,  though  it  did  not 
wane,  seemed  not  so  much  as  before  for  the  sake  of  mere  free  move- 
ment, which  was  now  a  matter  of  course  to  her,  not  an  active 
pleasure  in  itself.  She  went  about  picking  flowers,  dragged  her 
little  wagon  about,  and  chased  the  cats,  as  before;  she  was  all  the 
year  as  fond  as  ever  of  running  away  in  joke  when  called,  —  a  trick 
that  in  time  had  to  be  stopped,  — and  of  playing  chase,  or  "bear," 
a  more  dramatized  form  of  the  same  thing:  but  in  all  this,  the  mere 
bodily  activity  became  more  and  more  subordinated  to  the  mental 
element.  There  were  many  ebullitions  of  high  physical  spirits,  in 
which  running,  frisking,  jumping  were  mere  overflows  of  muscular 
exuberance,  —  but  this  also  was  a  different  thing  from  the  enjoy- 
ment of  the  movements  for  their  own  sakes,  as  interesting  attain- 
ments, in  the  year  before.  She  still  loved  to  go  on  rambles,  and 
the  distances  to  which  she  would  wander  off,  if  one  left  her  to 
choose  the  road,  increased:  she  was  apt,  however,  to  have  some 
objective  point,  as  the  neighbor's  farmyard,  the  creek  that  crossed 
the  ranch  a  half  mile  away.  This  last  walk  (a  favorite  one  from 
the  thirty-second  month  on)  is  the  longest  I  have  recorded  during 
the  year,  and  in  this,  or  even  in  shorter  ones,  she  would  ask  to  be 
carried  from  time  to  time,  —  almost  always  on  rough  ground.  She 
was  willing  to  be  wheeled  in  her  carriage,  or  taken  driving,  instead 
of  insisting  on  her  own  feet  (this  I  note  in  the  thirty-fifth  month 
and  afterward). 

She  could  still  be  tripped  now  and  then:  I  have  one  note,  in  the 
twenty-ninth  month,  of  her  falling,  on  rough  ground.  Later,  in  the 
thirty-fifth  month,  she  discovered  that  she  could  stand  on  one  foot, 
and  called  my  attention  to  it :  she  was  interested  in  doing  it,  and 
could  keep  her  balance  for  five  or  six  seconds,  which  indicates  a 
pretty  complete  mastery  of  equilibrium  in  standing. 

Climbing  continued  an  interest  in  itself,  and  her  ability  to 
climb  increased:  indeed,  as  every  year  offered  more  difficult  feats 
in  climbing  to  her,  as  she  grew  more  skilful,  this  power  never  alto- 
gether ceased  to  afford  the  interest  of  novelty.     On  the  stairs  she 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  383 

insisted  on  walking.  I  note  a  remonstrance  in  the  twenty-sixth 
month,  when  her  mother  wished  to  cany  her  downstairs:  "I  walk 
all  the  time!"  By  the  end  of  this  month,  she  was  really  able  to  go 
up  and  down  alone  without  the  least  oversight,  and  I  refer  to  her 
coming  upstairs  to  me  as  a  matter  of  course.  Although  my  notes 
are  not  clear  on  the  point,  it  is  my  impression  that  there  was  still 
an  effort  to  follow  her  in  these  trips,  for  safety's  sake;  but  in  the 
twenty-seventh  month  I  have  several  notes  of  her  running  away 
and  coming  upstairs  quite  alone.  She  was  in  the  habit  of  having  a 
chat  with  me  in  the  dark  in  my  room  every  evening  between  dinner 
and  bedtime,  and  I  have  several  notes  of  her  rambling  off  by  herself 
before  I  was  ready,  and  going  up  alone  in  the  dark  to  my  room. 
Certainly  before  the  end  of  the  twenty-seventh  month  we  let  her  go 
up  and  down  freely  alone,  and  she  was  as  secure  on  the  stairs  as  a 
grown  person. 

Her  acrobatics  on  the  train  in  the  twenty-eighth  month,  — 
climbing  over  the  back  of  a  seat,  and  up  to  the  window  ledge,  to 
fling  herself  backward  into  our  laps  —  have  been  mentioned  (p.  207). 
In  the  twenty-ninth  month,  she  climbed  over  a  stack  of  boxes  with  a 
good  deal  of  skill  and  persistence:  in  the  thirtieth  I  note  a  disposition 
to  climb  anything  that  offers  footing,  —  trees  with  low  branches  (that 
is  to  say,  a  few  inches  from  the  ground),  ladders,  carpenters'  horses, 
etc.  I  observed  in  this  month  that  her  method  of  climbing  was 
just  the  same  as  that  of  an  older  person,  pulling  up  with  her  hands 
and  putting  her  knee  on  the  surface  she  wished  to  reach:  she  had 
used  the  knee  thus  from  the  very  first  effort  to  climb  a  step.  Once 
in  this  month  she  slid  boldly  down  from  some  mattresses,  piled 
high  on  a  bedstead,  to  a  chair,  too  far  below  for  her  feet  to  touch. 
At  another  time,  she  climbed  into  a  tree  for  some  hard  and  worth- 
less fruits,  which  I  allowed  her  to  gather  to  play  with  :  she  got  up 
a  foot  or  so  from  the  ground,  and  labored  for  a  long  time  to  reach 
as  many  as  possible  of  the  little  fruits,  coming  down  to  put  them 
into  my  pocket,  and  climbing  up  again,  and  stretching  herself  up  as 
far  as  she  could  to  reach  them.  Although  at  this  time,  as  a  rule, 
rather  averse  to  hard  exertion,  she  would  pull  and  tug  valiantly 
to  climb.  From  the  thirty-third  month  she  was  fond  of  climbing 
up  into  the  high  judge's  seat  above  the  tennis  court.  In  the  thirty- 
fourth,  in  camp  in  the  Siena,  one  of  her  chief  joys  was  to  climb 


384  University  of    California.  [Vol.  1. 

over  the  rocks  and  logs,  and  she  was  ambitious  and  entirely  fearless 
in  this:  she  would  tug  up  the  rocks,  saying  at  each  step,  "Higher 
yet!  higher  yet!"  like  Longfellow's  youth.  She  was  dressed  in 
trousers  and  overalls,  which  helped  her  climbing. 

Jumping  did  not  become  a  common  feat.  I  have  notes  from 
the  twenty-ninth  month  of  her  "jumping  about"  in  high  spirits, 
but  I  do  not  know  just  what  the  movement  referred  to  was,  — 
probably  not  strictly  jumping.  In  the  thirty-second  month,  she 
practised  jumping  from  the  lounge,  evidently  a  little  afraid,  yet 
desirous  of  doing  it;  later,  holding  my  hand  to  steady  herself,  she 
did  it  very  well.  Again  in  the  thirty-fifth  month  she  had  a  spell  of 
practicing  at  jumping  from  a  step. 

She  still  liked,  now  and  then,  to  fall  back  on  the  earlier  and 
simpler  motions  and  positions.  I  note  a  fondness  for  rolling  about 
on  a  bed  in  the  twenty-fifth  month  (and  doubtless  at  other  times 
that  I  have  failed  to  note:  in  this  year,  as  the  matters  to  be 
recorded  grew  more  complex,  I  could  not  keep  complete  record  of 
anything  but  steps  in  advance  in  the  principal  movements).  In  the 
thirtieth  month  I  note  her  rolling  about  the  floor,  and  add  that  this 
rolling  about,  flat  on  back  or  side,  seems  to  afford  her  now  and 
then  great  physical  comfort,  and  that  she  is  at  all  times  very  ready 
to  lie  down  on  the  floor.  Rolling  on  the  lawn  in  the  same  month, 
he  begged  me  to  join  her:  "You  ain't  too  big.  I  will  show  you, 
how  to  roll  over  and  over.  Just  this  way."  She  could  not  steer 
herself  at  all,  and  when  asked  if  she  could  roll  to  any  given  place, 
she  would  try  it,  and  shout,  "  Yes!  I  can  roll  to  the  jasmine!"  or 
say,  "  No,  —  I  can't,"  according  as  she  had  happened  to  bring  up 
in  the  desired  region  or  not.  She  amused  herself  once  in  the 
twenty-seventh  month  by  creeping  upstairs,  as  she  had  done  at 
first  (and  indeed,  at  seven  years  old  she  will  do  this,  as  well  as 
experimenting  in  all  other  possible  ways  of  getting  up  and  down 
stairs,  — ■  sliding,  hitching,  etc.). 

I  observed  in  the  thirtieth  month  that  she  still  sat  flat  on  the 
floor  with  legs  turned  outward  at  the  knees  ;  at  no  such  angle  as  in 
the  first  year,  but   in   a  position  that  I  found  impossible  myself. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  3^5 

The  position  is  to  be  obtained  by  resting  on  hands  and  knees,  and 
then  lifting  the  body  backward  to  a  sitting  position  ;  but  it  requires 
a  flexibility  of  the  leg  joints  possessed  by  few  adults.  The  child 
finds  it  difficult  at  seven  years,  but  can  do  it  (see  p.  340). 


In  the  latter  part  of  the  twenty-first  month,  I  was  told  that 
when  supported  in  deep  water  to  swim,  the  child  paddled  like  a 
dog  some  twenty  feet:  but  this  I  did  not  see,  nor  was  there  ever 
opportunity  to  repeat  the  experiment  till  she  was  nearly  six  years 
old;  at  this  time  she  showed  no  instinctive  ability  to  swim,  but  was 
quick  in  learning  to  balance  her  body,  and  would  probably  have 
learned  to  swim  with  a  few  lessons. 


INSTINCTS  CONNECTED  WITH  FOOD-TAKING. 

Sucking,  Licking,  Smacking,  Biting,  Spitting  Out. 

Sucking,  if  it  is  to  be  classed  as  an  instinctive,  not  a  reflex, 
movement,  is  the  most  perfect  and  probably  the  earliest  instinct  in 
the  human  infant.  That  it  is  properly  an  instinct,  and  not  a  reflex, 
I  think  we  must  admit.  As  Preyer  points  out,  although  the  move- 
ment is  perfectly  performed  at  birth  upon  the  proper  peripheral 
stimulus,  the  touch  of  a  suitable  object  between  the  lips,  yet  it  may 
begin  without  the  peripheral  touch  (perhaps  by  central  initiation,  as 
in  dreaming) ;  instead  of  being  quickly  exhausted,  like  other  reflexes, 
it  goes  on  continuously  till  hunger  is  satisfied,  then  stops,  without 
any  change  in  the  pressure  on  the  lips  having  taken  place,  —  indeed, 
if  the  infant  is  not  hungry,  sucking  may  fail  to  follow  at  all  on  the 
touch  of  the  nipple,  which  may  be  rejected  from  the  mouth  instead, 
of  being  laid  hold  on  and  sucked;  and  instead  of  becoming  more 
firmly  consolidated  in  its  reflex  character  with  repetition,  sucking 
loses,  as  we  grow  older,  the  traits  of  a  reflex  that  it  at  first  pos- 
sessed, and  becomes  purely  voluntary. 

This  movement  appeared  in  the  case  of  my  niece  as  soon  as 
she  was  put  to  the  breast,  and  was  vigorous  and  perfect.  In  the 
early  weeks,  objects  accidentally  put  into  her  mouth  were  sucked, 
but  this  of  course  rarely  happened  before  she  could  guide  her 
hands  to  her  mouth.  From  the  fourth  week,  throughout  the  second 
month  and  into  the  third,  she  had  a  habit,  if  held  against  one's  face 
when  hungry,  of  laying  hold  on  the  cheek  with  her  lips,  and  sucking 
frantically.  At  two  months  old,  she  would  suck  the  cheek  if 
hungry,  but  if  not  hungry  would  put  her  mouth  on  it  and  lick  it. 
This  is  the  earliest  mention  I  find  of  licking;  but  on  its  first  appear- 
ance the  movement  was  well  co-ordinated  and  definite.  (Thefeeling 
with  the  tongue  about  the  lips,  in  the  seventh  week,  had  of  course 
resembled  licking,  but  was  a  much  more  indefinite  movement.) 

In  the  third  month  appeared  the  marked  enjoyment  of  sucking 

as  a  soothing  sensation,  —  first  in  the  case  of  sucking  the  thumb, 

then  in  the  use  of  the  rubber  nipple,  associated  for  more  than  a  year 

with  going  to  sleep  (above,  pp.  265-7).     In  tlle  same  m°"th,  suck- 

(386) 


shin-n.1  The  Development  of  a   Child.  387 

ing,  and  also  smacking  the  lips,  were  expressions  of  hunger;  but  in 
the  latter  part  of  the  fourth  the  baby  frequently  pursed  her  lips  and 
made  sucking  and  smacking  sounds,  without  any  connection  with 
hunger,  or  any  reason  that  I  could  discern.  (In  the  eighth  month 
and  again  in  the  fifteenth  there  was  a  marked  habit  of  smacking 
the  lips,  ■ — ■  beginning  in  both  cases  over  the  eating  of  sugar,  and 
continuing  for  several  days:  the  child  would  begin  smacking  vigor- 
ously every  now  and  then,  and  keep  it  up  for  some  seconds.  In 
the  eighth  month  this  would  be  done  without  apparent  suggestion 
or  object,  but  in  the  fifteenth  month  the  smacking  was  clearly  a 
sign  of  desire  for  food.)  As  she  lay  asleep  at  sixteen  weeks  old, 
I  saw  her  repeatedly  sucking,  though  she  had  been  fed  shortly 
before  she  fell  asleep. 

In  the  middle  of  the  fourth  month,  the  baby  instead  of  sucking 
a  finger  put  into  her  mouth,  began  to  bite  down  on  it  with  her 
toothless  jaws,  quite  persistently.  After  this,  her  rattle  handle,  rub- 
ber rings,  etc.,  were  not  only  sucked  but  also  chewed  and  mumbled 
with  her  jaws.  The  sucking  alone  still  gave  much  contentment, 
and  I  find  it  mentioned  as  an  accompaniment  of  tranquil  attention; 
but  the  notes  of  biting  increase.  In  the  nineteenth  week,  she 
began  the  trick  above  described  (Muscular  Sensation,  p.  189)  of 
holding  the  rubber  of  her  rattle  between  her  jaws,  and  jerking  it 
out.  Besides  biting  down  hard  on  a  finger,  she  would  now,  when 
lifted  to  one's  face,  bite  at  chin  and  cheek,  instead  of  sucking  or 
licking.  Objects  that  offered  pretty  firm  resistance  were  preferred 
to  softer  ones,  both  for  biting  and  for  sucking:  e.  g.,  a  wooden  tip 
on  one  of  her  rattles  was  preferred  to  a  rubber  ring  attached  to  it 
and  intended  for  biting. 

It  was  evident  that  the  new  disposition  to  bite  was  connected 
with  dentition.  The  teeth  were  now  growing  up  through  the  gum, 
and  the  irritation  of  their  growth  must  have  been  the  stimulus  to 
the  movement:  after  the  appearance  of  the  first  pair,  at  twenty-two 
weeks  old,  there  seems  to  have  been  less  disposition  to  bite,  and 
indeed,  after  this  I  have  but  one  note  of  biting  (viz.,  that  at  twenty- 
eight  weeks  old  she  bit  a  hole  through  a  rubber  nipple)  during  the 
rest  of  the  sixth  month  and  the  whole  of  the  seventh.  The  biting 
action  may  have  served  to  allay  some  irritation  in  the  gums,  yet  I 
26 


38b  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1. 

cannot  think  that  it  was  accidentally  hit  upon,  and  then  continued 
for  the  sake  of  the  relief  it  was  found  to  afford:  it  began  with  a 
definiteness  and  persistence  that  made  it  seem  highly  instinctive  in 
character;  the  baby  was  impelled  to  it  without  visible  reason,  as 
the  time  for  its  usefulness  approached.  Moreover,  as  the  teeth 
began  to  press  close  under  the  skin,  and  the  baby  was  apt  to  hurt 
herself  in  biting,  she  still  persisted  in  the  movement. 

After  the  appearance  of  the  teeth  and  the  cessation  of  biting  just 
spoken  of,  notes  of  sucking  become  again  more  frequent.  In  the 
last  week  of  the  sixth  month  the  baby  put  her  mouth  to  my  cheek, 
sucking  at  it,  and  also  licking  and  mouthing,  as  she  used  to  do  at 
two  months  old.  Later  in  the  same  week  (179th  day)  she  had  been 
playing  with  some  oranges  in  a  dish,  and  ended  by  putting  her 
mouth  down  to  the  dish,  and  sucking  and  licking  it,  then  began  to 
lick  and  mouth  the  oranges,  putting  her  head  down  to  them;  in 
this,  she  was  evidently  finishing  a  hand-investigation  of  their 
properties  by  appealing  to  the  tactile  sense  of  lips  and  tongue.  She 
was  a  little  disposed  for  a  week  or  two  in  the  eighth  month  to  suck 
her  thumb. 

With  the  close  of  the  seventh  month,  I  find  once  more  notes  of 
biting.  At  seven  months  old,  in  sucking  bits  of  orange,  the  baby 
crushed  them  with  her  teeth,  and  a  few  days  later  (214th  day)  set 
her  teeth  into  loquats  given  her  to  suck.  She  evidently  had  in 
this  no  idea  of  eating,  nor  even  of  dividing  anything  with  her  teeth : 
even  ten  days  later,  at  thirty-two  weeks  old,  when  she  had  some- 
what persistently  bitten  in  two  some  pieces  of  orange  that  were 
held  to  her  mouth  to  be  sucked,  she  seemed  puzzled  at  feeling  the 
divided  half  in  her  mouth,  and  had  no  idea  of  swallowing  it.  Soon 
after  this  there  began,  together  with  evidences  that  more  teeth  had 
started,  another  period  of  noticeable  disposition  to  bite  and  chew 
(probably  thirty-third  week,  —  first  noted  in  the  thirty-fourth)  and 
it  appeared  decidedly  to  stimulate  the  baby's  deficient  appetite 
to  be  able  to  chew  at  something,  as  a  bit  of  dried  beef  or  a  bread- 
crust;  and  from  this  time  on,  the  desire  to  masticate  in  connection 
with  food   became  more  and    more   imperative1   (see   pp.    331-2). 

'One  must  hesitate  in  questioning  the  verdict  of  those  competent  medical 
authorities  who  object  to  any  food  that  requires  mastication  before  the  appear- 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  3§9 

Given  loquats,  she  would  promptly  bite  them  in  two,  and  could  no 
longer  be  trusted  to  suck  at  them  for  the  juice  (thirty-sixth  week). 
She  bit  at  all  sorts  of  objects,  sometimes  hard  ones,  as  a  bottle, 
hurting  her  sensitive  gums.  The  third  tooth  came  through  in  the 
thirty-sixth  week,  and  three  more  in  the  thirty-ninth.  Having  now 
both  upper  and  lower  incisors,  the  baby  began  to  masticate  properly, 
so  far  as  the  incisors  alone  sufficed,  and  was  eager  to  do  it :  her 
chewing  was  no  longer  the  blind,  instinctive  movement  it  had  been 
earlier,  and  was  aptly  joined  with  swallowing  movements.  Dry 
crackers  were  preferred  to  bread-crusts,  perhaps  because  they 
crunched  more  crisply  under  the  teeth,  and  were  chewed  and 
swallowed  neatly.  By  the  end  of  the  eleventh  month  —  the  baby 
having  now  seven  teeth  — ■  a  bit  of  steak  was  chewed  and  swallowed, 
instead  of  being  sucked  ;  and  when  a  piece  too  tough  to  tear  with 
the  teeth  was  given  her,  she  was  annoyed,  and  refused  to  suck  it. 
Pretzels  only  she  would  suck  and  lick,  being  chiefly  interested  in 
the  salt  surface.  At  the  end  of  the  twelfth  month,  she  would  bite 
through  the  skin  of  an  apple,  taking  out  bits  of  skin  and  flesh,  as 
skilfully  as  a  grown  person,  so  that  apples  could  no  longer  be 
trusted  in  her  hands  to  be  rolled  about  and  played  with. 

She  still  sucked  and  chewed  at  uneatable  objects  that  got  into 
her  hands,  though  both  tricks  declined  as  she  came  to  require  more 
complex  and  mental  interest  from  objects  than  the  mere  investiga- 
tion of  their  surfaces. 

There  was  a  recurrence  of  seizing  at  our  faces  as  we  held  her, 
and  putting  her  lips  to  them,  sometimes  biting  (tenth  and  eleventh 
months),  but  licking  and  sucking  no  longer  appeared  at  such 
times;  and  there  was  a  visible  tendency  toward  differentiation  in 
the  eleventh  month,  the  applications  of  the  mouth  becoming  expres- 
sions of  friendliness,  out  of  which  kissing  developed,  while  the  biting 
was  done  in  rough  play,  and  connected  with  snatching  at  the  hair, 

ance  of  the  molars  :  yet  it  is  impossible  not  to  wonder  if  they  do  not  underrate 
the  significance  of  the  strong  instinct  to  masticate  food  which  appears  with  the 
incisors,  perhaps  a  year  earlier.  I  find  the  suggestion  backed  by  the  opinion  of 
Dr.  Joseph  Le  Conte  ;  and  also  by  that  of  some  experienced  family  physicians. 
There  is  no  doubt  that  the  weight  of  authority  among  specialists  in  the  care  of 
children  is  against  it. 


39°  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

etc.1  The  baby  would  begin  with  the  caressing  touches,  then  end 
in  biting,  as  if  the  opportunity  had  suggested  the  use  of  her  teeth; 
and  there  was  always  something  instinctive  and  non-volitional  about 
the  action,  in  spite  of  its  connection  with  other  manifestations  of  a 
spirit  of  rough  romping,  —  it  seemed  as  though  she  felt  somehow 
impelled  to  use  her  teeth.  This  trick  recurred  at  intervals  in  the 
second  year,  being  noted  every  month  up  to  the  sixteenth,  after 
which  it  disappeared  for  months,  reappearing  in  the  middle  of  the 
twenty-first  month,  persisting  throughout  the  month,  then  disap- 
pearing again  (not  without  a  little  discipline),  until  the  latter  part  of 
the  twenty-seventh  month,  when  for  a  couple  of  weeks,  extending 
into  the  twenty-eighth  month,  I  find  the  trick  often  mentioned. 
Late  in  the  thirtieth  month,  it  returned  with  peculiar  force,  and  for 
about  three  weeks  was  persistent.  The  child  would  rush  up  to  a 
person  and  laughingly  set  her  teeth  in,  like  a  romping  puppy. 
Reproof  seemed  only  to  infix  the  disposition,  by  a  sort  of  converse 
suggestion,  and  set  her  to  doing  it  again,  defiantly,  yet  gayly,  and 
without  a  particle  of  ill  temper.  Before  the  end  of  the  thirty-first 
month,  this  impulse  passed  away,  or  yielded  to  discipline;  but  in 
the  thirty-fifth,  among  sundry  methods  of  misbehavior  toward  her 
kitten,  there  was  for  about  a  week  a  persistent  impulse  to  bite  it; 
she  probably  hit  upon  this  by  accident,  as  she  was  cuddling  the 
kitten  to  her  face,  when  it  suddenly  occurred  to  her  to  put  its  head 
into  her  mouth  and  bite  it,  till  the  kitten  cried  out.  We  stopped 
this  —  or  it  passed  away  independently  of  our  efforts  —  in  a  few 
days;  but  once  in  the  next  week  she  could  not  resist  putting  the 
tip  of  the  kitten's  tail  into  her  mouth  and  biting  it.  After  this, 
though  she  remained  disposed  to  rough  play,  and  biting  as  well  as 
other  roughness  did  reappear,  there  were  no  more  such  noticeable 
fits  of  impulse. 

The  child's  mother  says  that  sportive  biting  was  a  habit  of  her 
own  at  perhaps  four  years  old,  and  was  broken  up  with  difficulty: 
as  far  as  she  can  recall  her  feelings,  she  acted  under  an  irresistible 
physical  impulse,  and  felt  keen  physical  satisfaction  in  the  sensation 

'It  is  worth  noticing  that  the  word  bite  was  at  first  (sixteenth  month)  con- 
fused with  kiss;  even  in  the  eighteenth  month  the  words  seemed  to  be  occa- 
sionally confused  with  each  other,  and  with  "eat,"  but  this  may  have  been 
in  play. 


Sins.s-j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  39r 

of  biting  down  on  some  one's  skin;  there  was  no  ill  temper  at  all  in 
it.  During  the  month  when  biting  seemed  most  persistent  and 
uncontrollable  in  my  niece,  I  was  disposed  to  connect  it  with 
dentition,  noticing  that  at  the  time  she  had  her  thumb  in  her  mouth 
a  good  deal,  biting  it,  or  feeling  her  gum  (not  sucking  it).  But  a 
comparison  of  dates  throughout  fails  to  show  any  constant  connec- 
tion between  the  growth  of  teeth  and  the  periods  of  disposition  to 
bite.  It  is  likely  that  such  a  connection  existed,  but  was  impos- 
sible to  trace,  because  the  earliest  growth  of  the  teeth,  far  below 
the  gum,  could  not  be  fixed  by  date.  Certainly  there  was  no 
relation  between  the  dates  of  their  appearance  through  the  skin  and 
the  biting.1     Nor  was  the  trick  simply  part  of  the  child's  general 

]I  did  not  keep  a  careful  account  of  the  appearance  of  the  teeth,  but  find 
incidental  notes  of  most  of  them.  It  may  be  of  some  interest  to  append  the 
record,  though  I  suppose  that,  apart  from  the  relation  of  dentition  to  health, 
spirits,  food,  and  such  small  instinctive  habits  as  the  one  above  described 
(which  relation  I  have  already  pointed  out  in  the  proper  places),  this  record 
has  about  as  little  developmental  interest  and  value  as  anything  that  could  be 
noted  by  the  observer  of  an  infant;  besides  being  a  subject  on  which  further 
record  is  not  needed,  since  physicians  of  infancy  have  already  very  complete 
records  of  the  process  of  dentition.     However,  for  what  it  is  worth,  I  append  it: 

154th  day  (22  weeks  old),  lower  first  incisor. 

156th  day  (23d  week),  lower  first  incisor. 

250th  day  (36th  week),  upper  first  incisor,  right. 

269th  day  (39th  week),  upper  first  incisor,  left. 

2j2d  day  (39th  week),  lower  second  incisor,  right. 

2j2d  day  (39th  week,  upper  second  incisor,  right. 

294th  day  (42  weeks  old),  lower  second  incisor,  left. 
?  (nearly  a  year  old),  upper  second  incisor,  left. 

446th  day  (15th  month),  upper  first  molar,  right. 

489th  day  (16  months  old),  upper  first  molar,  left. 

About  533d  day  (iSth  month),  eyetooth. 

535th  day  (iSth  month),  lower  first  molar. 
?  (tgth  month),  lower  canine,  left. 

566th  day  (19th  month),  eyetooth. 

578th  day  (19  months  old),  lower  canine,  right. 

6o2d  day  (20th  month),  lower  first  molar. 

27th  month,  latter  half,  2  second  molars. 

The  other  two  milk-teeth  were  not  yet  cut  in  the  thirty-first  month,  when  I 
fancied  their  growth  might  have  something  to  do  with  the  child's  persistent 
biting;  but  I  have  no  record  of  their  appearance. 

In  the  thirty-first  month,  the  dentist  discovered  a  tiny  hole  in  one  of  the 
molars,  too  minute  to  be  filled;  and  on  the  child's  third  birthday  he  filled  two 


392  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

romping,  coming  and  going  as  the  fits  of  rough  play  did ;  for  there 
were  many  periods  of  strong  disposition  to  rough  play,  with 
slapping,  snatching  at  our  hair,  etc.,  without  any  appearance  of  the 
impulse  to  bite. 

From  the  first,  the  baby  thrust  the  nipple  from  her  mouth  quite 
neatly  with  her  tongue,  when  she  was  satisfied.  She  had  no  further 
ability  to  spit  anything  out,  however,  and  when  she  first  tasted  a 
novel  liquid,  on  the  214th  day  (about  seven  months  old)  she  merely 
dropped  her  jaw  and  let  it  trickle  out.  About  a  month  later  when 
her  greater  freedom  of  movement  enabled  her  to  get  many  small 
foreign  bodies  into  her  mouth,  as  bits  of  paper,  flower  petals,  etc., 
which  we  would  at  once  take  out,  she  began  to  assist  us  by  getting 
the  object  to  the  tip  of  her  tongue:  in  the  tenth  month,  when  told 
to  thrust  such  things  out  of  her  mouth,  she  would  get  them  cleverly 
to  the  tip  of  her  tongue,  scraping  the  roof  of  her  mouth  with  a 
comical  little  sound,  and  put  her  tongue  out  to  let  us  take  the  object 
from  it.  By  the  end  of  the  twentieth  month,  she  could  spit  out 
objects  (as  the  stones  of  loquats)  easily. 

Other  Instinctive   Movements. 

Pushing  with  the  feet  began  very  early  (pp.  1  So,  189,  note),  and 
if  not  reflex,  was  highly  instinctive,  probably  having  some  antici- 
patory connection  with  the  development  later  of  the  locomotor  and 
balancing  movements,  —  under  which  head  instances  of  the  move- 
ment will  be  found  (p.  334).  Pushing  and  pulling  with  the  hands 
were  among  the  earliest  movements  following  on  the  acquirement 
of  grasping;  indeed,  they  were  to  some  extent  involved  in  the 
vague  fumblings  that  preceded  grasping.  Like  creeping,  they  did 
not  seem  in  a  high  degree  instinctive  movements;  they  were  rather 
selected  ones,  from  among  the  many  vague  ones  used  in  the  baby's 
early  random  manipulations  of  objects;  there  must  have  been  a 

small  holes,  leaving  two  others  as  too  small  to  be  operated  on  yet.  This  is 
probably  unusually  early  for  the  milk-teeth  to  show  decay,  —  almost  upon  their 
appearance,  —  and  the  child  inherits  bad  teeth  on  her  father's  side:  but  even 
though  hers  is  an  unusual  case,  it  is  safe  to  draw  the  general  moral  that  inspec- 
tion by  a  dentist  can  hardly  be  begun  too  early. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  393 

certain  predisposition  to  them,  if  only  in  their  necessary  relation  to 
anatomical  structure;  but  I  saw  little  of  that  immediate,  unacquired 
adaptiveness  in  the  movements,  or  strong  impulse  toward  them, 
which  characterized  the  true  instincts.  The  one  exception  was  the 
case  of  pulling  with  the  arms  to  raise  herself  (see  Sitting,  p.  328), 
which  did  seem  instinctive.  One  may  see,  also,  a  certain  instinctive 
element  in  the  great  fancy  for  pulling  against  resistance  (Muscular 
Sensation,  pp.  189-90). 

In  the  eleventh  and  twelfth  months,  the  mere  exercise  of  these 
elementary  manipulations,  pushing  and  pulling  objects  about,  made 
up  a  surprisingly  large  part  of  the  baby's  "play,"  and  afforded  a 
really  remarkable  amount  of  interest  and  pleasure,  —  pulling  down 
books  from  shelves,  opening  and  shutting  doors,  etc.  In  the 
second  year,  in  larger  variety,  much  of  her  play  was  still  to  be 
analyzed  into  the  mere  use  of  these  movements,  —  as  in  dragging 
about  a  little  wagon,  etc. 

Kicking,  as  an  instinctive  movement,  having  adaptiveness,  did 
not  appear  early.  The  mere  muscular  exercise  of  kicking  was 
common  in  the  early  months  (pp.  187-9);  and  kicking  the  legs  up 
instead  of  making  walking  motions  has  already  been  mentioned  in 
the  account  of  the  movements  of  locomotion.  Throwing'  one  leg 
out  in  a  purely  imitative  way,  when  told  to  "kick,"  was  done  with 
some  skill  from  the  fifteenth  month  (how  the  movement  origi- 
nated, whether  spontaneous  or  taught  the  child  in  sport  by  some 
one,  I  do  not  know);  and  up  to  thirty  months  old,  the  word  was 
still  understood  only  thus  intransitively:  the  idea  of  kicking  an 
object  had  not  appeared.  Kicking  her  heels  backward,  against  a 
seat  she  was  sitting  on  (twenty-second  month),  or  even  swinging 
them  forward  (against  the  prop  of  her  nursery  chair  tray,  e.  g.)  I  do 
not  count  as  true  kicking.  So  far  as  I  know,  she  never  saw  any 
one  kick  an  object,  and  when  the  movement  did  appear.it  must 
have  been  quite  instinctively.  The  first  record  I  have  of  it  is  on 
the  683d  day,  twenty-third  month,  when  the  child  was  seized  with 
an  impulse  to  kick  a  toad,  insisting  that  she  "must  kick  toad," 
and  after  this,  she  occasionally  showed  desire  to  kick  small  ani- 
mals. It  was  one  of  her  methods  of  misbehavior  toward  her  kitten, 
in  the  latter  half  of  the  third   year.     There  did   not  seem  to  be 


394  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

animosity  in  it,  certainly  not  anger;  the  source  of  the  apparently 
uncontrollable  impulse  was  hard  to  define.  Kicking  never  entered 
into  her  rough  play  with  us. 

Slapping  and  striking,  however,  was  from  the  twelfth  month,  a 
common  action  in  this  roguish  roughness.  At  intervals  during  the 
whole  of  the  second  and  third  year,  the  child  would,  when  taken  in 
arms,  slap  merrily  at  our  faces,  snatch  at  our  hair,  etc.;  by  the  end 
of  the  nineteenth  month  she  would  strike  with  a  stick,  and  later 
it  was  the  presence  of  a  stick  in  her  hand  that  seemed  to  bewitch 
her  to  strike  and  bang  with  it,  —  not  at  people  so  often  as  at  any 
object  that  she  could  make  a  good  bang  on,  as  the  furniture ;  but 
even  as  late  as  the  thirty-sixth  month,  and  on  into  the  fourth  year, 
I  have  notes  of  her  striking  people  merely  because  she  found  a 
convenient  stick  in  her  hand  (pp.  196-7).  In  the  thirty-fourth 
month,  she  slapped  her  kitten  sometimes,  without  discernible 
motive.  I  find  but  a  single  note  of  her  ever  striking  in  anger: 
this  was  when  her  grandmother  held  her  still  to  be  wiped,  when 
she  was  in  romping  spirits  after  her  bath  (thirty-fifth  month). 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  eleventh  month,  the  trick  of  stiffening 
the  body  in  resistance  to  being  taken  out  of  the  bath,  was  acquired. 
This  seems  to  have  been  purely  instinctive,  for  the  baby  could 
hardly  have  known  that  it  made  it  more  difficult  to  lift  her:  it  may 
have  been  more  an  expressive  than  resisting  movement,  however. 
In  the  thirteenth  and  fourteenth  month  (as  has  been  related  under 
Muscular  Sensation,  p.  185),  if  not  interested  when  some  one  was 
holding  her,  or  not  desiring  to  be  lifted,  she  would  stiffen  and 
wriggle  away,  or  else,  in  the  case  of  being  lifted,  would  raise  her 
arms  and  relax  all  her  muscles  limply,  so  that  she  would  slip 
through  the  grasp,  —  also  a  highly  instinctive  device,  it  would 
appear.  I  note  this  slipping  out  of  the  hands  now  and  then  after- 
wards; but  wriggling  away  was  the  commonest  form  of  resistance; 
a  sharp  twisting  away  was  observed  in  the  eighteenth  month. 
Direct  pulling  back  was  never  noticed.  The  child  appeared 
instinctively  to  feel  the  insufficiency  of  her  strength  in  such 
resistance. 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  395 

The  development  of  sniffing  has  already  been  mentioned,  under 
Smell.  This  movement,  beginning  in  the  early  months  as  a  mere 
muscular  exercise,  or  an  expression  of  gayety,  was  later  associated 
with  smelling  by  deliberate  teaching,  and  had  no  marked  instinctive 
character;  though  it  was  doubtless,  like  many  other  movements, 
acquired  more  readily  because  of  predisposition  to  the  act. 


Some  other  movements,  as  throwing,  might  be  considered  so 
completely  race  movements,  so  certain  to  be  taken  up,  through 
sheer  predisposition  to  them,  by  every  child,  that  they  should  be 
classed  here:  but  as  a  matter  of  fact,  these  are  acquired  imitatively, 
or  even  taught,  and  I  do  not  wish  to  extend  the  range  of  instinct 
too  widely.  Certainly  it  does  not  seem  wise  to  include  under  it 
any  movements  that  involve  the  use  of  a  tool,  even  so  simple  a  tool 
as  a  missile,  —  though  I  have  necessarily  spoken  of  striking  with 
a  stick  in  connection  with  striking  with  the  hand.  I  may  add  that 
of  all  forms  of  using  a  weapon  or  tool,  this  striking  with  a  stick 
seemed  the  most  instinctive,  —  far  more  so  than  throwing. 

In  many  other  movements  besides  those  here  described,  an 
instinctive  element  is  very  plain.  Many  expressive  movements,  as 
laughing,  are  purely  instinctive.  On  the  other  hand,  in  most  of 
the  instinctive  movements,  as  walking,  the  constant  use  of  volition 
and  adaptive  intelligence  was  strikingly  evident.  It  is  as  impossible 
to  distinguish  sharply  between  instinctive  and  deliberate  action 
as  between  reflex  and  instinctive.  I  must  classify  according  to  the 
element  in  the  action  that  seems  to  predominate,  or  to  be  of  the 
more  importance  and  interest. 

Again,  the  presence  of  instinct  affecting  rather  the  central  than 
the  motor  activities  was  often  to  be  detected:  there  were  strong 
instinctive  elements  in  the  emotions,  for  instance.  These  I  shall 
call  attention  to  when  I  come  to  them,  but  shall  not  attempt  to 
disentangle  from  their  more  obvious  connections,  to  class  them  in 
the  present  chapter. 


In  all  the  types  of  movement  described  in  the  foregoing  pages, 
race  inheritance,  not  individual  intelligence,  has  been  the  control- 


396  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

ling  element.  The  gap  between  these  and  ideational  movements, 
in  which  the  interest  rests  not  in  the  movement  itself  but  in  the 
central  activity  which  it  expresses,  is  so  wide  that  it  does  not  seem 
to  me  logical  to  group  them  together  in  a  classified  record.  I 
close  the  subject  of  Movement,  therefore,  with  the  following  sum- 
maries, leaving  intelligent  movements  to  be  recorded  under  cate- 
gories belonging  to  the  Intellect. 


SUMMARY    AND    TABLES    RELATING   TO    THE    NON- 
IDEATIONAL   MOVEMENTS. 

I. 

Spontaneous  Movements  :'   Characterized  by  entire  absence  of 
purposiveness:  — 

Table  i.     Spontaneous  Movements. 


i.  Purely  spontaneous  movements.  Due  to  dif- 
fused stimulus  from  growth  of  lower  motor  centres. 
Gradually  inhibited  by  co-ordinated  movements, 
but  surviving  somewhat  in  sleep  for  several  years. 


ist  to  3d  months. 

[Mrs.  Moore,  isttosth; 
Preyer,  ist  to  7th.] 


2.  Similarly  irregular  movements,  not  purely 
spontaneous  but  due  to  diffused  stimulus  from 
heightened  general  sensation.  Passing  under  ordi- 
nary conditions  into  regular  expressive  movements, 
but  surviving  always  as  irregular  e.vcess  move- 
ments of  expression. 


ist  to  3d  months. 
[Preyer,  ist  to  7th.] 


3.  Accompanying  movements,  due  to  diffused 
excess  of  voluntary  motor  stimulus,  or  strain  of 
attention. 


From  6th  week. 

[Mrs.  Moore,  from  9th 
week ;  Preyer,  from  8th 
month.] 


1  Dr.  Mumford's  recent  ingenious  theory,  which  treats  these  as  survival 
movements  dating  back  to  the  aquatic  ancestry  of  man,  must  not  be  entirely 
ignored  here.  The  obvious  objection  to  the  theory  is  the  enormous  antiquity 
of  the  survival :  it  seems  incongruous  that  at  a  stage  of  development  when 
structure  is  of  a  distinctly  primate  type,  and  the  muscular  condition  reveals 
traces  of  the  arboreal  stage  (as  Dr.  Robinson  has  plausibly  shown),  the  pre- 
vailing movements  should  lapse  back  to  a  stage  so  inconceivably  more  remote. 
Even  if  the  theory  be  accepted,  it  does  not  dispose  of  the  classification  of  these 
movements  as  spontaneous  in  origin  ;  it  simply  accounts  for  the  form  they 
take,  as  the  diffused  stimulus  passes  into  the  easiest,  because  most  ancient 
channels.  But  many  of  the  movements  —  as  the  asymmetric  movements  of  the 
eyes,  the  grimacing,  the  rolling  of  the  head — certainly  can  not  be  brought 
under  the  theory  at  all. 

(  397  ) 


398 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


II. 

Reflex  Movements:  Involuntarily  purposive.  Following 
immediately  on  peripheral  sensation,  without  necessity  of  central 
action,  and  along  well-established  paths  (Preyer). 


Table  2.     Commoner  Reflexes. 


EARLIEST  DATE  OF 
APPEARANCE. 

MOVEMENT. 

OBSERVER. 

LATER  DATES  OF 
APPEARANCE. 

(Swallowing1  ...  \ 
(Crying.. j 

Many  observers. 

(  4th  day  —  Sully. 
\  5th  day  —  Preyer. 
(  Very  early— Shinn. 

f  3d  day  —  Moore. 
J  3d  or  4th  —  Shinn. 

1  Champneys f 

|  1st  fortnight  — 
[     Darwin. 

J  1st  week  —  Tracy. 

1st  day —  5th  day.. 
1st  day  —  1st  week 

\  28th  day  —  Shinn. 

f  1st  week  —  Tracv. 
\  BySoth  day— Shinn 

f  1st  week  — Tracy. 
\  42d  day  —  Shinn. 

f  Before  426  dav  — 

I     Shinn. 

42d  day  —  Shinn. 

("  23d  day  —  Shinn. 
<  Before  49th  day  — 
(     Moore. 

J  2^d  day —  Shinn. 
\  44th  day  —  Hall. 

Throwing  up  arm'. 

Rubbing  eves 

(With  yawning  and 
stretching.) 

Shinn. 

1  Prenatal,  according  to  Preyer. 

2  Prenatal,  according  to  Champneys.     Preyer  speaks  of  it  as  usual  in  the 
•newborn,  but  does  not  report  it  in  his  own  observations  till  5th  day. 

3  Classed  by  Tracy  as  "impulsive,"  or  spontaneous. 

*  Purposiveness  is  not  very  evident  in  such  reflexes,  but  they  seem  survivals 
of  escaping  or  defensive  movements,  retaining  still  a  certain  character  of  the 
sort,  while  the  definiteness  of  their  character  and  their  immediate  relation  to 
peripheral  stimulus  distinguishes  them  clearly  in  actual  observation  from  the 
mere  overflow  movements  classed  as  "spontaneous." 

5  Not  the  mere  closing  of  eyes  at  bright  light,  which  may  be  seen  from  the 
first. 


Shinn. 


The  Development  of  a   Cliild. 
III. 


399 


Instinctive  Movements:  —  Purposive,  in  part  consciously,  in 
part  unconsciously.  Hereditary  and  racial,  but  developed  largely 
under  influence  of  individual  experience  and  volition. 

1.  Purely  instinctive:  —  Sucking,  licking,  biting,  smacking, 
thrusting  out  objects  with  tongue;  turning  head;  striking,  kicking 
(at  objects);  stiffening,  relaxing,  or  wriggling  away  (to  escape  a 
hold);  pulling  against  resistance;  paddling  to  keep  afloat  in  water; 
walking  movements. 

2.  Developed  by  effort,  following  on  strongly  instinctive  impulse:  — 
Holding  up  head;  sitting,  pulling  self  to  sitting  position,  or  raising 
body  by  abdominal  muscles;  kneeling,  standing,  walking,  running, 
climbing. 

3.  Selected  movements,  but  determined  largely  by  instinct:  — 
Grasping;  raising  self  to  sitting  position  sidewise  or  backward; 
rolling,  creeping,  rising  to  knees,  sitting  on  heels;  spitting  out; 
sniffing;  pushing  and  pulling. 

4.  Imitative,  and  scarcely  instinctive  at  all:  — Jumping. 


Table  3.     Development  of  Grasping :    Transition   from   mouth 
to  hands  as  grasping  organ. 


MOUTH    AS    GRASPING 

HANDS   AS    GRASPING 

ORGAN. 

ORGAN. 

1st  month. 

1st  3  weeks. 

Special  sensibility  (passive) 
in  lips  and  tongue. 
Sucking  instinct. 

Reflex  clasping. 

Tendency   of    hands   to   p 
getting  to  mouth. 

renatal    position,   accidentally 

4th  week. 

5th  week. 

2d  month. 

7th  week. 

Seizing  with  lips  anil  suck- 
ing a  surface  brought  in  con- 
tact with  them. 

Groping  movements   with 
head,  to  aid  mouth  grasp. 

Appearance  of  active  touch 
in  tongue. 

9th  week. 

Precocious  effort  to  carry 
accidental  movement. 

object  to  mouth,  repeating  an 

Signs  of  special  sensibility 
(passive)  in  finger-tips. 

400 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


Table  3. — Continued. 


3d  month. 


10th  week. 


1 2th  week. 


13th  week. 
4th  month. 

14th  week. 
15th  week. 
16th  week. 


At  16  weeks, 
(113th  day.) 

17th  week. 


5th  month. 

iSth  week. 
19th  week. 


.MOUTH    AS   GRASPING 
ORGAN. 


HANDS   AS   GRASPING 
ORGAN. 


Longer  clasping 

reversed. 


Thumb 


Efforts  to  set  hands  to  mouth. 

Hands  carried  constantly  to  mouth,  sucked  and  mum- 
bled. 

Hands  closed  mechanic- 
ally on  objects  touched. 
Objects  in  hands  carried 
unintentionally  to  mouth 
with  hands :  association 
forming  between  the  move- 
ment, and  touch  sensations 
in  lips  and  tongue. 

Thumb  carried  to  mouth  at  will  and  sucked. 
Readied   by    diving  head 
down  as  much  as  by  carry- 
ing thumb  up. 

Appearance  of  active  touch 
in  fingers. 

Longer  and  more  or  less 
conscious  clasping.  Pulling 
to  raise  self.  Fumbling  about 
for  objects,  and  picking  up 
deliberately  when  touched 
either  by  palm  or  back  of 
hand,  — first  hand  grasping, 
by  feeling  only. 
Deliberate  efforts  to  put  objects  in  mouth. 


Objects    when    felt    often  Growing  skill   in  picking 

sought  with  head,  to  grasp      up  objects.    Both  hands  used 
with  mouth.  for  heavy  object. 

Growing  skill  in  putting  objects  in  mouth.  Yet  relation 
of  hand  and  arm  movements  to  touch  sensations  in  mouth 
still  imperfectly  understood,  as  shown  by  blunders. 

Objects    seen,   then    fum- 


Attempts  to  grasp  with 
mouth  or  tongue  by  visual 
guidance  (119th  day). 


bled  for,  and  grasped  when 
touched,  —  approach  to  true 
"rasping. 

First  attempt  to  grasp  by 
visual  guidance  (118th  day). 


Grasping  with   mouth,  by  Slow      improvement     in 

diving  head  at  object,  more  grasping, — still  largely  grop- 
frequent  and  skilful  than  ing,  and  grasping  on  con- 
grasping  with  hand.  ,   tact. 

Objects  grasped  solely  for  purpose  of  getting  them  to 
mouth. 


Shinn.' 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


401 


Table  3.— Concluded. 


20th  week. 
21st  week. 


6th  month. 


7th  month. 
8th  month. 


9th  month. 
10th  month. 


MOUTH    AS   GRASPING 
ORGAN. 


HANDS   AS   GRASPING 
ORGAN. 


Head  put  down  to  grasp 
only  when  object  does  not 
move  readily  under  hands. 

.Mouth-grasp  disappearing. 


Great  advance  in  skill. 
Simultaneous  grasping  with  hands  and  mouth. 
Mouth  put  to  object  after  the  clutch  with  hands. 
Simple   manipulation   of    object,   instead   of   immediate 
putting  to  mouth. 

Seizing  toes,  with  skilful 
co-operation  of  ankle. 

Usually  both  hands  used, 
object  cautiously  inclosed 
between  them  Practically 
no  errors  in  distance  or 
direction. 

Grasping  now  the  chief 
and  absorbing  interest,  — 
everything  reached  for. 

More  varied  handling, 
greatly  increased  skill :  e.  g., 
heavy  glass  marble,  1% 
inches  diam.,  held  in  one 
hand,  and  ball,  2  inches,  in 
the  other.  Yet  still  clumsy, 
thumb  not  always  reversed, 
etc. 

Toes  carried  skilfully  to 
mouth. 

Decline  in  interest  in 
grasping,  superseded  by 
interest   in   locomotion. 

Swift  snatching  appears. 

Grasping  with  fmger-tips 
quite  imperfect. 

Finger-tips  more  skilful; 
pin  picked  up  with  ease; 
single  hair  played  with. 

Right  forefinger  tip  dif- 
ferentiated for  delicate  inves- 
tigation. 


Objects  less  promptly  car- 
ried to  mouth,  sometimes 
not  at  all. 


Objects   rarely   carried   to 
mouth.1 


Table  4.     Priority  of  Mouth  in  Touch  and  Prehension. 


STAGE  OF  DEVELOPMENT 


Special  passive  sensibility 

Grasping,  on  contact 

Groping,  to  grasp  

Active  touch 

Grasping 


MOUTH. 


In  lips  and  tongue,  1st  week 

With  lips,  4th  week 

With  head,  yt/i  week 

With  tongue,  yth  week 

With  mouth,  by  head- 
movement,  more  fre- 
quent and  skilful,  up  to 
loth  week 


In  lingers,  oth  zveek. 

With  hands,  /oth  week. 

With  hands,  12th  week. 

With  fingers,  12th  -week. 

With  hand,  by  arm-move- 
ment, more  frequent  and 
skilful,  after  20th  week. 


1  Recurrence  of  practice  two  or  three  times  in  the  second  year. 


4-02  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

Table  5.     Sequence  of  Stages  to  Hand-Grasping. 

1.  Reflex  clasping. 

2.  Hands  often  fell  by  lips  and  tongue,  in  connection  with  certain  involun- 
tary arm-movements. 

3.  Renewal  of  these  sensations  sought  by  voluntary  repetition  of  the  arm- 
movements. 

4.  Sensations  increased  and  varied  by  touch  of  objects,  accidentally  brought 
to  mouth,  after  being  mechanically  clasped. 

5.  Deliberate  groping  for  and  laying  hold  on  familiar  objects,  in  familiar 
place,  to  carry  to  mouth  (i.  e.,  repetition  of  movements  that  have  often  resulted 
in  accidentally  carrying  objects  to  mouth:  extension  of  the  association  series). 

6.  Hands  and  objects  seen,  while  grasping  takes  place;  formation  of  visual 
association  also. 

7.  Grasping  at  objects  visually  located,  for  sole  purpose  of  carrying  to 
mouth. 

s.  ('.rasping  for  varied  purposes,  manipulation,  inspection,  etc.,  as  associa- 
tions increase  in  number  and  variety. 

It  will  be  seen  that  my  record  of  the  development  of  grasping 
completely  confirms  Preyer's  in  the  following  respects:  — 

(1)  The  origin  in  (a)  reflex  clasping;  (I?)  tendency  of  the  hands 
to  move  upward,  accidentally  reaching  the  mouth;  (V)  the  sucking 
instinct. 

(2)  The  slow  growth  (a)  of  mechanical  holding  to  an  object 
once  clasped,  the  thumb  gradually  becoming  reversed  (a  stage 
which  I  saw  by  the  ninth  week,  however,  Professor  Preyer  not  till 
the  fourteenth);  and  (/;)  of  laying  hold  on  objects  accidentally 
touched. 

(3)  The  final  appearance  of  true,  visually  guided  grasping  at  the 
end  of  the  fourth  month,  —  even  in  the  same  week,  the  seventeenth, 
in  which  he  saw  it. 

(4)  The  slow  growth  afterward  of  good  co-ordination  between 
thumb  and  fingers,  and  neat  grasping. 

I  can  not  find,  however,  that  he  observed  at  all :  — 
(1)  The  persistent  voluntary  carrying  of  hands,  and  afterward  of 
accidentally  clasped  objects,  to  the  mouth,  before  true  grasping,  — 
an  action  which  according  to  my  analysis  above  was  an  essential 
stage  in  forming  the  association  series  that  brought  about  true 
grasping. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  403 

(2)  The  period  of  fumbling  for  objects,  and  grasping  purposely 
but  without  visual  guidance,  —  a  period  that  lasted  a  month  in  the 
case  of  my  niece,  and  brought  the  motor  and  tactile  associations 
involved  in  grasping  to  considerable  perfection,  before  the  visual 
element  was  added. 

(3)  The  appearance  of  active  touch  about  tongue  and  lips,  and 
efforts  to  seize  with  the  mouth,  which  in  my  record  clearly  precede 
active  touch  and  prehension  in  the  hands. 

With  regard  to  these  three  points  of  difference:  — 
1.  Mrs.  Moore's  and  Mrs.  Hall's  records  agree  with  Professor 
Preyer's  and  mine  in  recognizing  the  early  movement  of  hands  to 
face  and  mouth,  and  the  early  clasping,  as  involuntary;  and  in 
noticing  the  period  of  gradually  longer  and  more  deliberate  clasping 
that  preceded  true  grasping.  They  also  confirm  my  observation 
of  a  period  of  carrying  the  hands  to  the  mouth  with  unmistaka- 
ble intention,  before  any  intentional  grasping  was  observed.  No 
observer  but  myself,  however,  seems  to  have  noticed  any  distinct 
period  of  carrying  to  the  mouth  objects  held  in  the  hand  (at  first 
those  involuntarily  clasped  upon  contact,  and  afterward  those 
groped  for  in  familiar  places),  before  true  grasping:  but  Sully  has 
several  notes  implying  some  such  period;  Darwin  places  the  habit 
of  carrying  things  to  the  mouth  so  early  (twelfth  and  thirteenth 
weeks)  that  it  probably  preceded  grasping;  and  both  Mrs.  Moore 
and  Mrs.  Hall  place  the  first  true  grasping  so  early  (eleventh  week 
and  twelfth  week)  that  it  seems  probable  they  classed  as  true 
grasping  an  early  type  which  I  agree  with  Preyer  and  Vierordt 
in  thinking  only  apparent.1  If  the  latter  view  is  correct,  Mrs. 
Moore's  observation  that  this  early  stage  passed  into  the  later 
one  through  a  period  in  which  the  hand  was  watched  while  grasp- 
ing took  place,  confirms  mine  and  my  analysis  above  (Table  5) 
in  a  striking  manner.  In  the  case  of  my  niece,  others  about  the 
child  believed  that  she  was  really  grasping  with  intention  and 
visual  guidance  some  time  before  I  did.  If  my  analysis  in  Table  5 
is  correct,  grasping  is  largely  an  empiric  act,  reached  through  con- 

1  The  Senses  and  the  Will,  p.  246. 
27 


404  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

secutive  series  of  associations.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  the  first 
visually  guided  grasping  comes  without  some  such  succession  of 
stages,  it  must  be  regarded  as  much  more  purely  instinctive  than 
in  my  interpretation.  I  shall  show  in  a  later  publication  the  bear- 
ing that  this  question  has  on  my  observations  as  to  the  visual  per- 
ception of  space  and  form. 

2.  The  noticeable  period  of feeling for  objects  and  grasping  upon 
contact,  in  the  case  of  my  niece,  was  doubtless  due  to  the  fact  that 
she  was  from  the  twelfth  week  propped  among  cushions  in  a  chair, 
with  convenient  objects  on  the  tray  before  her,  where  she  learned 
to  expect  to  find  them.  To  this  gradual  acquirement  of  muscular 
co-ordinations  was  also  due,  perhaps,  the  caution  and  accuracy  of 
her  grasp  from  the  first,  and  the  use  of  both  hands,  while  Preyer's 
child  often  estimated  distance  and  direction  wrong,  and  for  weeks 
used  only  one  hand. 

3.  As  to  the  mouth-grasp,  it  may  have  been  an  individual  trick 
in  the  case  of  my  niece,  or  may  be  an  essential  stage  in  develop- 
ment, overlooked  by  other  observers;  the  question  should  be  settled 
by  farther  observation.2  Professor  Preyer,  though  an  almost  infal- 
lible observer,  was  so  prepossessed  by  the  idea  of  taste  association 
in  his  interpretation  of  the  early  importance  of  the  mouth  in  con- 
sciousness, that  he  may  have  failed  to  notice  some  significant  indi- 
cations of  its  part  in  touch  and  prehension.  But  traces  of  such 
a  stage  appear  in  his  record,  and  in  those  of  several  other  observers: 
his  child  brought  the  mouth  down  to  an  object  that  did  not  move 
readily  under  the  hands,  just  as  my  niece  did;  Mrs.  Moore's  groped 
for  the  breast  with  the  head,  in  the  second  week,  and  afterward,  in 
the  ninth,  made  reaches  for  it  with  head  and  neck;  while  Sully's,  in 
the  nineteenth  week,  made  efforts  to  seize  a  biscuit  with  the  mouth. 

The  following  table  will  show  how  my  observations  of  the  stages 
of  development  in  hand  grasping  compare  chronologically  with 
those  of  others.  I  enter  in  it  only  such  stages  as  are  reported  by 
other  observers  besides  myself. 

2  As  these  pages  go  to  press,  I  find  that  I  had  overlooked  Ti'edemann's  dis- 
tinct note  of  a  passing  habit  of  mouth  grasping, confirmed  by  Perez;  but  neither 
observer  assigns  it  to  a  definite  place  in  the  development  of  grasping. 


Shinn  j 


The  Development  of  a   Child. 


405 


J3 

nt 
H 

"rt 
u 

'5b 
o 

*o 
c 
o 

u 

A 

o 

u 
> 

« 

o. 

E 
o 
U 


C 

'a 

(0 
h 

o 


3 

crj 


2 

•  c+£ 

> 

:         :  <^  Z    :    :         1         :    :         :    : 

X 

?    ^  _«-*    *    ;         t         ;     :         1     : 

< 

C 

:        i  «  J?  :    :        :        ':    :        !    ! 

•     . 

> 

:         :      -^    I    I         I         :     :         :    : 

H 

.       '.      pi;       1       i           '.1 

^ 

:        :      £    :    :        •        I    :        :    • 

W 

ffl 

:     •   :      "&  :    :        :        I    :        :    : 

_ 

•           ;      i     i           •         en     •      •           •                             •      ; 

^ 

:        :       0    :    :         :        :    :        :    : 

a 

1      '.     a  '.   '.       '          '      '   ' 

^j 

:        :       «   :    .        :        :    :        :    . 

c/5 

I     ;    <  \  '■     i     ;  !     ;  ; 

_ 

:         .   'f ;.    :    :    :         .         :     :         ■    ; 

a 

1         i  2    :    :    :         ■         •    :      ^    :' 

0 

:  S  «   :   :       :       : 'g      %  g 

:       or:        :        :  2       -  ° 

0 

£* 

:> 

:  -r  t    :    :         :         :  tt      w  10 

£*• 

;     ^     ^!    ;    :     ^j     ^    |       :    • 

j 

j 

:j 

(/3 

:       -h        on   ■    ■      0        on   ;         •     ■ 

1-1       >-«                r\i       h.    .         , 

j 

'  ^i 

I      it 

I            • 

:        :     ^    j    :     ^        :    :      g    ! 

:  s    :      S        : 

t         r        *■*::        "*          ::              : 

K 

:  —    :      -G        I 

:        :      -5    1    :      -B        :    :      -5    : 

.        .       n    .    .       _        :    :       r,    : 

:  a.  :      c» 

CH        .        .              M                          .              M 

:^'^ 
.  5  s 

4. 

'■    ':    :      •*        i    :        :J 
:    :    :      j=        :    :        ' _ 

0     ■ 

E   ! 

■J, 

4-t                    .                               .      ^ 

a, 

:  -t  h 

CN 

:    :    :       i^       •    •        ■  ^ 

0   ■ 

■  -  — 

M               • 

.      .      .           M             .                         ro 

u 

"°  J*    :         !      ^    :    :      ^         i    i         :    : 

ii.M 

0 

j  ;  S      ^ts^l^        :      JS5      £        :g        :: 

■J   5   &   S 

0 

^ 

~  ™  C4       0  -^  x  "  0        •      ™  "~-  h       c^i        :  -i 

".  X    X     -T 

NV  H         —  C          Om           .        *c   •-   *-         >-«           .(N           .* 

PO  m     ^-     p^-. 

^i^^'H^'H^-                 ■   :  J2       •       ■■       :  -jfi 

2 

<u  cd  u  =  >  a  >^_^      Jd      .*.*•£      ^      ^  ^       J  ij 

.CC-C-S  °«  «-5£     5     55"     -5    55     if.— 

^   0  tfi^ 

£ 

*.  ~  ^j: 

72 

^^     t; 
•^  —  x  c 

103\CJ\0          i-<           » — <    ( — «           i-<           i-.i—^,           M           "-H           IN(N 

N»  to^- 

:  0    . 

~r"  • 

.v. 

i    15   i£    1    :■=    !    :a 

■    •  cu    -a    ■    •  ■£    •    •  cu 

:>,  ; 

"■J       : 

'.'■2~  :>• 

_d    i 

•  "i3    • 

<±   m    :  0    :^j 

bo  :  u    :  c    :    ; «-    ;    :  a- 

a    •  0    •  "    •    .  0    •    •  ra 

:  0    : 
:  °-  : 

5    0    •       ■  en 

"ft  1 

0    v    :  to  ■  0 
u    c    :  e    :  a. 

■~  c  ~    :  ■£    :    :       :   :-o 

u, 

.2*  : 

—  D        ; .«    :    :  -a    :    • 

:  D 

■2.    ■ 
■u  b! 

CCJ   a 

:  a  : 

-2  a    j 

"!=      c   -  w     '  D. 
3     a  rt  g    : 
"3    T3  -^  ifi   :  i2 

l"1  *M  i?^'- 

j/-  u»Bad    !  S  a  3  en 

.- ja  u  x  bfj—    :    .  a  a  c 

•0  t3  m  is  s  ii  o2« 

S  -.a  a  5  0  has  -  bt  — 

-1 

_a„_ 
"35  ^ 

^  "O  ho  fi     '• 

:  .a  ^  c 
O  U  M  | 

L-  ,"v.     —     1— 

^-  ^   -  — 

g<°    j^i      2=--:='S^2   =   §•  w  Y  £  Spiv's   C*3   c 

«<J 

•j;    ^ 

<■-    u 

sOO     ^5     o,S     OS 

> 

:: 

arts  a 


1 

u 

c 

0 

^ 

O 

X 

CO 

< 

4) 

i- 

'  ™ 

11 
— 

r~. 

U 

- 

%, 

"- 

M 

^ 

rt 

a; 

cu-o 

j- 

0 

_ 

0 

- 

Q 

tn 

E 

1* 

1 

a; 

? 

c 

c 

u 

— 

u 

1 

5 

ib 

S 

fc« 

OJ 

- 

j: 

a 

j: 

_^ 

hf 

~ 

0 

,a 

~ 

tn 

_ 

<r. 

X 

r.r 

a 

- 

--/. 

- 

t/1 

c 

fi 

- 

V 

= 

- 

— 

X, 

- 

n 

a; 

- 

c 

— 

4_, 

^ 

'= 

0 

- 

OJ 

hn 

Tl 

c 

a 
0 

-; 

^. 

c; 

u 

t/; 

r" 

bfl 

~ 

a; 

0 

Jj    u 

u 

JJT3 

'/ 

-    3 

*5 

M  u  JS 

^ 

=  x^ 

fciyiS 

B*-s 

406 


!  diversity  of  California. 


[Vol.  i 


Table  7.     Movements  of  Equilibrium  and  Locomotion : 
Chronological  Succession. 


1st  Month. 

1st  week 


2d  week. 

3d  week. 
4th  week. 

2d  Month. 

5th  week 

6th  week. 
7th  week 
9th  week. 


TURNING 
HEAD. 


Instinctive 
or  spontane 
ous  rolling  of 
head . 


3d  Month. 

nth  week. 


12th  week. 
4th  Month. 


More  ap 
pearance  of 
volition  in 
turning  head 
to  look. 


Head  turned 
freely  at  will. 


Head  first 
turned  while 
balanced. 


BALANCING 
HEAD. 


Special  in- 
nervation of 
neck  mi 
cles. 

First  efforts 
to  lift  head. 


Head  turned 
freely  at  wi 
while     ba 

.mi  i'd. 


Head  first 
balanced 
erect. 


SITTING. 


OTHER   MOVEMENTS. 


Head  bal 
a  need  per 
fectly. 


Desire  for 
upright  posi 
tions  and  ef 
fort   to   hold 
back  erect. 


Efforts    to 
raise  self. 


Sitting  with 
support  be- 
comes habit- 
ual position 


[The  power  of  turning  and  balancing  the  head  having  been  attained  for  life- 
early  in  the  third  month,  I  drop  these  two  columns  from  the  table.] 


Shi.nn.] 


The  Development  of  a    Child. 
Table  7. — Continued. 


407 


SITTING. 

ROLLING. 

CREEPING. 

OTHER    MOVEMENTS. 

5th  Month. 

Sitting  with 

19th  week. 

less  and  less 
support,    till 

Turning 
over    from 

the  slightestib  ac  k     to 

steadying    is'side. 

20th  week. 

enough. 

Effort  to  strain  forward  and  reach  ob- 

6th Month. 

ject,  — first  attempt  at  locomotion. 

Sitting  with- 

24th week. 

out  support, 

Rolling 

Approxi- 

with    slowly 

over    from 

mate  creep- 

increas ing 

back     to 

ing    move- 

security     of 

stomach . 

ment,  with- 

balance. 

out     inten- 

7th Month. 

Secure  sit- 

tion. 

27th  week. 

t  i  n g    as    a 

28th  week. 

rule,  but  oc- 
casional fall. 

Rolling 
from  stom- 
ach to  back 

Rolling 

30th  week. 

over     a  n d 
over  freely. 

KNEEL- 

STAND- 

WALK- 

ING. 

ING. 

ING. 

8th  Month. 

32c)  week. 

Pulling  self  to 
knees  and  partly  to 

33d  week. 

Random 
creeping 
position 
and  move- 
ments   fre- 
quent. 

Drew  self 
forward, 
flat      on 
stomach. 

feet. 

• 

Great  and  joyous  ac- 

34th week. 

Acquire- 

tivity  in   free  kicking, 

ment  of  sun- 

rolling, twisting  about. 

Daily 

35th  week. 

dry  methods 

with  random  creeping 

pulling  to 

of  sitting  up 

movements. 

feet. 

9th  Month. 

at  will. 

Creeping 

Rising 

backward. 

freely    to 

36th  week. 

Rolling 

First  true 

knees 

First 

from     now 

creeping. 

without 

wal  king 

on    a b a n- 

support. 

m  0  v  e- 

doned. 

ments. 

37th  week. 

Medley  of    positions  and  movements,  sitting,  kneel- 
ing, creeping,  scrambling,  pulling  to  feet,  &c. 

[The  only  farther  progress  under  the  head  of  sitting  was  the  learning  to  sit 
down  backward,  in  the  15th  month,  and  rolling  was  abandoned,  so  I  drop  these 
two  columns  from  the  table.  ] 


4o8 


University  of    California. 
Table  7. — Continued. 


[Vol.  1. 


10th  Month 

40th  week. 


41st  week. 
42d  week. 

43d  week. 

44th  week. 

11th  Month 

45th  week. 


46th  week. 
47th  week. 
48th  week. 


12th  Month 

50th  week. 

51st  week. 


CREEPING. 


KNEELING. 


STANDING. 


Disappearance  of  varied  movements 
by  selection  of  best  adapted,  especially 
standing  and  creeping. 


Rapid  and 
free  creep- 
ing;. 


Creeping 
on  hands  and 
feet,  rather 
than  hands 
and  knees, 
to  the  end  of 
the  year. 


Dropping 
from  feet  to 
knees,  hold- 
ing to  sup 
port. 


Constant  ef- 
forts to  stand 
alone. 

Stan  ding 
alone  a  few 
seconds. 


Rising  to 
feet  without 
help. 

Standing 
a  lone     s 
curely. 


WALKING. 


CLIMBING. 


Stepping 
along,  hold 
ing    to    sup 

port. 


Walks  led 
by  one  hand, 
but  no  incli- 
nation to  do 
it. 


Letting 
self  down  by 
hands,  from 
chair. 


Pulling 
self  up  step. 


Climbing 
up  and  down 
stairs.  Ef- 
forts to 
climb  boxes, 
chairs,  etc. 


Incessant  joyous  activity,  standing  up  and  sitting  down,  edg- 
ing, creeping,  and  climbing  about. 

Bold,  free, 
and  constant 
A  few  steps  climbing 
alone,  at  first  throughout 
coaxed,  at  the  month, 
last  volunta- 
rily^ 


[Kneeling  and  standing  had  now  been  for  some  two  months  well  acquired 
(it  remains  only  to  note  that  standing  on  one  foot  was  seen  in  the  29th  month)  ; 
and  as  creeping  was  abandoned  during  the  thirteenth  month,  I  drop  these  three 
columns  from  the  table  for  the  second  year.] 


Shinn.1 


The  Development  of  a   Child. 
Table  7 — Concluded. 


409 


13th  Month 
53d  week. 


54th  week. 
55th  week. 


Uth  Month 
15th  Month 

16th  Month 
17th  Month 

18th  Month 


19th-24th 
Months 


3d  Year 


Walking  free- 
ly, steadied  by 
finger. 

Across  a  room 
alone. 

Walking  alone 
with  secure  bal- 
ance, turning, 
bending,  step- 
pins  over  slight 

bstacles. 

Toddling  free- 
ly about  gar- 
den, hundreds 
of  feet. 

Incessant  tod- 
dling about. 


WALKING. 


CLIMBING. 


Clim./ing  per- 
petually in- 
creases in  skill 
and  variety 
t  h  rou  g  h  out 
month. 


Gait  increased 
when  eager  to  a 
sort  of  trot. 


Almost     runs 
ten    eager, — 
clumsy  trot.  Perpetual  de- 

Trotting  a  Sire    to    crjmrj, 

great  deal.  j 

»"•"««  j  and   growing 

Walking    almost     abandonedj        .    ? 
for  the  trotting  gait.  ability:     in- 

Walking  back-  creased  heights 

ward.  ,   onstantly 

Stepping     up  rlimhed 

and   down,—    T 
downstairs 
without  help. 

Stepping  up- 
stairs without 
help. 

Incessant  and  joyous  activity 
Distances  up  to  mile  or  more 
Still  occasional  tripping.  Ladders  from 

21st  month. 


Running  about 
untiringly,  with 
joy  and  exulta-      Into  chairs. 
tion.     Still  trips      Into     high 
often.  chair. 


Last  note  of  tripping  in  29th 
month. 


Unflagging 
zeal  in  climbing. 
Trees  climbed 
30th  month. 


Jf.MPING. 


Imitative     ef- 
forts to  jump. 


Acquired  at 
23  months,  and 
zealously  prac- 
ticed. 

Never  a  com- 
mon movement: 
jumping  down 
occasionally 
practiced 


4 1 0  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

The  diagrams  on  the  opposite  page  summarize  the  chronological 
succession  of  the  movements  very  briefly,  and  compare  my  results 
with  those  of  the  other  observers  who  have  recorded  at  all  fully 
the  process  of  acquiring-  these  powers.  Most  observers  give  only 
the   date  of  acquirement  of  each  movement  recorded  by  them.1 

I  have  tried  to  supplement  these  diagrams  with  a  comparative 
table  of  such  dates  as  are  afforded  by  the  reports  of  other 
observers :  but  so  many  of  these  are  unavailable  for  comparison 
(for  the  reasons  given  in  the  foot-note1),  that  I  abandoned  the 
effort  after  any  complete  comparison,  and  contented  myself  with 
a  mere  table  of  extreme  dates  (Table  8),  which  will  give  an  idea 
of  the  range  of  variation  so  far  recorded.  It  should  be  noted, 
however,  that  in  several  cases  earlier  dates  are  reported  than  are 
given  in  the  table,  but  not  in  such  terms  that  I  can  be  certain  of 
the  stage  of  development  really  meant :  thus,  for  example,  two  of 
the  Talbot  papers  report  children  as  "  sitting  alone  "  at  five  months 
old,  but  I  cannot  tell  whether  this  means  that  secure  sitting  alone 
for  minutes  together,  on  a  perfectly  level  surface,  was  really  attained. 

The  table  shows  plainly  that  the  American  children  were  more 
active  in   acquiring   the    earlier  movements:    the  full  text  of  the 

1  Such  bare  dates  are  of  little  use  even  for  mere  comparison  of  the  periods 
of  completion  in  development,  because  in  the  gradual  process  each  observer 
may  set  down  a  different  stage  as  marking  practical  completion.  He  records, 
for  instance,  "Sat  alone  at  six  months  old."  Does  this  mean  that  the  child  for 
the  first  time  succeeded  in  balancing  for  a  second  or  two,  or  that  sitting  with 
secure  balance  was  acquired  as  a  habit  for  life?  In  the  case  of  my  niece,  three 
months  intervened  between  these  two  stages.  Did  the  child  sit  on  a  cushion, 
lap,  or  other  yielding  surface,  affording  even  the  slightest  support  about  the 
buttocks,  or  on  a  flat,  hard  surface  ?  My  record  shows  two  weeks  between  these 
stages,  and  Mrs.  Wood's  a  month,  while  Preyer  also  noticed  that  sitting  on  the 
hard  level  was  a  more  difficult  and  later  achievement.  Does  "standing  alone  " 
refer  to  the  first  successful  balancing,  or  to  secure  equilibrium  for  minutes  at  a 
time  ?  Alter  the  former  stage  was  attained,  it  took  Mrs.  Hall's  boy  two  months 
to  reach  the  latter;  my  niece  required  three  months,  and  Preyer's  child  not  less 
than  eight  months,  the  whole  process  of  learning  to  creep,  walk,  and  run 
coming  in  between.  Even  the  order  of  succession  of  the  movements  is 
valueless  for  comparative  purposes  when  we  do  not  know  what  stage  of  the 
process  of  acquirement  the  note  refers  to ;  this  is  evident  by  a  glance  at  the 
diagrams,  with  reference  to  the  question  whether  creeping  came  before  or 
after  kneeling,  for  instance,  in  my  record,  or  standing  in  Preyer's  and 
walking  in  Mrs.  Hall's. 


Shinn.] 


The  Development  of  a  Child. 


4II 


First  effort ortendency 

1,  Miss  Shinn's  record'  2,  Prof.  Preyer's;  3,  Mrs.  Hall's;  4,  Mrs.  Beatty's. 
The  dotted  lines  indicate  the  decline  and  disappearance  of  the  movement. 
The  broken  lines  show  that  the  record  was  incomplete.     In  a  few  cases, 
however  (as  "  rolling"  in  Prof.  Preyer's  record),  the  movement  itself  was  but 
partly  developed  by  the  child. 


4i: 


University  of  California. 


[Vol.  i. 


records  shows  them  on  the  whole  somewhat  in  advance  of  the 
European  children  in  standing  and  walking  (though  the  European 
children  began  earlier),  and  notably  so  in  climbing.  The  difference 
may  well  be  due  to  the  degree  of  freedom  allowed  in  dress  and 
position;  and  the  very  fact  of  restriction  in  the  earlier  movements 
may  hasten  the  beginning  of  the  later  ones. 

No  regular  difference  between  girls  and  boys,  in  strength,  pre- 
cocity, or  method  of  acquiring  the  movements,  can  be  detected 
from  any  evidence  so  far  attainable.  The  resemblance  in  these 
respects  between  an  American  girl  and  an  American  boy  seems 
closer  than  between  an  American  boy  and  a  German  boy.  But  the 
number  of  records  is  too  small  to  justify  any  generalization  here. 
It  is  a  reasonable  presumption  that  small,  light  children  will  be  earlier 
in  acquiring  balance  and  locomotion,  and  this  accords  with  the  gen- 
eral impression  of  mothers  and  nurses;  and  the  statistics  tend  to 
show  that  girls  are  smaller  and  lighter  than  boys  in  the  earliest 
years,  though  according  to  Dr.  Burk  (Growth  of  Children  in  Height 
and  Weight)  the  difference  is  not  marked  nor  conclusively  established. 

Table  8.     Extreme  Dates  of  Acquiring  Instinctive  Movements. 


MOVEMENT. 


Holding  head 
ist  effort 


EARLIEST    DATE    RECORDED. 


f  Wood                                     ) 
ist  day  -  Talbot   Papers,  case   E,    ^ 
I      two  children                       ) 
Habit nth  week,  Shinn 


Sitting — 

ist  effort ioth  week,  Wood  

Habit |26thweek    {sV/nnJ-  

Standing — 

ist  effort iSth  week,  Sigismund 

Several  min- 
utes alone  ...  40th  week,  Demme 7°t"  week,  Preyer 


LATEST  DATE  RECORDED 


.nth  week,  Preyer. 
.24  weeks,  Demme. 


iSth  week,  Hall. 

..11  months,  Demme. 

.14th  month,  Preyer.1 


Walking — 
ist  movements  19th  week,  Champneys  . 
Free  walking 
alone ...    8  months,  Sigismund  ... 


....41st  week,  Preyer. 
.24th  month,  Demme. 


1  Not  his  own  observation;  reported  from  "trustworthy  parents." 

The  following  appear  to  me  the  most  important  results  brought 
out  by  my  own.  record  of  the  movements  of  equilibrium  and  loco- 
motion, checked  by  comparison  with  other  records :  — 


shinn.j  The  Development  of  a   Child.  4r3 

i.  The  rudimentary  origin  and  gradual  development  of  some  of 
these  movements.  Thus,  holding  up  the  head  seemed  to  develop 
out  of  a  mere  special  innervation  of  the  neck  muscles,  at  times  of 
heightened  general  stimulus;  sitting  began  with  a  mere  stiffening 
of  the  back  against  a  support ;  creeping  defined  itself  out  of  a 
medley  of  experiments  in  moving  the  body  and  limbs  about.  In 
some  cases  the  steps  of  development  seemed  largely  empiric,  in 
others  instinctive,  and  in  some  cases  (notably  in  that  of  walking), 
the  first  sign  of  the  movement  seemed  completely  instinctive :  but 
on  the  whole,  careful  watching  seemed  to  reveal  a  more  gradual 
process  from  smaller  beginnings  —  to  analyze  the  movements 
down  into  simpler  elements,  so  to  speak  —  than  one  would  suppose 
from  casual  observation.  The  same  thing  seems  to  be  true  even  of 
the  very  precocious  instincts  of  young  animals,  Professor  Morgan 
and  Doctor  Mills  finding  some  little  development  from  imperfect 
beginnings  where  the  earlier  observers  had  seen  only  instincts 
innately  complete. 

I  do  not  find  that  any  other  observer  reports  quite  so  primitive 
beginnings  in  these  movements  as  I  do ;  but  I  suspect  this  is 
rather  because  the  observer  has  not  cared  to  push  his  record  so 
far  back  than  because  such  primitive  stages  did  not  occur. 

2.  The  marked  priority  of  the  movements  of  the  head,  trunk, 
and  arms  over  those  in  which  the  legs  took  part.  There  were  two 
distinct  periods,  —  one  (extending  through  the  first  seven  months)  in 
which  the  main  development  was  in  sense  perception,  together  with 
the  closely  correlated  power  of  grasping,  but  in  which  balancing 
and  turning  the  head,  sitting,  and  rolling,  followed  in  distinct 
succession;  and  another  (extending  through  a  half-year  more)  in 
which  the  main  activity  was  in  the  development  of  locomotion 
and  of  equilibrium  in  the  erect  position,  the  movements  developing 
alongside  each  other  with  much  less  distinct  order  of  succession. 

These  two  periods  are  clearly  evident  in  the  notes  of  other 
observers,  though  their  duration  varies.  In  all  the  records  we 
have,  the  movements  of  the  first  period  follow  in  strikingly 
similar  succession,  though  Mrs.  Hall  omits  record  of  balancing  the 
head,  and  Preyer's  child  apparently  was  never  given  the  oppor- 
tunity to  develop  rolling.  Mrs.  Beatty  and  Mrs.  Sharp  do  not 
record  the  earlier  periods  at  all. 


414  University  0/    California.  [Vol.  i. 

3.  The  late  and  empiric  development  of  creeping.  If  we  are 
to  regard  this  movement  as  a  survival  of  a  four-footed  mode  of 
progression,  it  is  curious  to  find  it  so  late,  after  sitting  and  rolling, 
even  after  standing  and  a  fair  degree  of  progress  in  walking :  yet 
all  the  observers  agree  as  to  this  tardy  development  of  creeping.1 
Moreover,  it  is  the  most  uncertain  and  variable  of  all  the  instinctive 
movements.  The  use  of  hands  and  knees,  which  is  more  common 
than  that  of  hands  and  feet,  is  perhaps  easily  accounted  for  by  the 
length  of  the  human  leg;  but  this  is  the  least  of  the  variations. 
My  niece  drew  herself  forward  with  her  elbows,  flat  on  her  stom- 
ach, before  she  could  creep;  Mrs.  Sharp's  eldest  boy  did  the  same; 
Preyer  says  of  the  first  creeping,  "  Can  creep,  or  rather  drag  himself 
along  somewhat."  Mrs.  Wood's  elder  boy  propelled  himself  "in 
measure-worm  fashion,"  and  her  younger  one  wriggled  along,  and 
later  drew  himself  forward  with  his  arms,  and  at  one  time  learned 
to  steer  himself  in  rolling,  and  used  that  as  a  means  of  voluntary 
locomotion  instead  of  creeping;  Mrs.  Hall's  boy  hitched  along  in  a 
sitting  position  before  he  could  creep.  Every  one  has  known  of 
babies  who  with  every  opportunity  never  creep  at  all,  hitching  or 
rolling  instead  ;  and  Dr.  Le  Conte  tells  me  of  instances  in  which 
the  creeping  is  tripedal,  one  leg  being  trailed  along  idle.  It  is 
curious  to  see  so  often  reported  an  involuntary  backward  move- 
ment (highly  displeasing  to  the  child,  whom  it  takes  directly  away 
from  the  objects  he  is  trying  to  reach),  immediately  preceding 
successful  creeping:  so  in  my  record  (above,  p.  340),  Mrs.  Beatty's 
(below,  p.  420)  and  Mrs.  Hall's.2  In  some  cases,  as  in  that  of  Mrs. 
Wood's  younger  boy  (below,  p.  424),  there  seems  to  be  a  strong 
instinctive  impulse  toward  proper  creeping;  but  on  the  whole  the 
impulse  is  vague,  urging  only  to  locomotion  of  some  sort,  in 
striking  contrast  to  the  definite  instinct  which  sets  the  baby's  legs 
mechanically  to  executing  the  alternate  motions  of  walking,  late 
and  difficult  though  that  movement  is  in  race  history. 

If  we  are  to  regard  creeping  as  a  survival,  its  late  appearance 
and  uncertainty  may  be  due  to  its  slight  usefulness  in  human  life, 

'Preyer  mentions  a  child  who  began  to  creep  in  the  fifth  month;  but  it  is 
not  his  own  observation,  and  I  know  <>f  no  other  case  so  early. 
2 Child  Study  Monthly,  Vol.  II,  p.  403. 


shi.nn.]  The  Development  of  a    Child.  4r5 

or  to  human  customs  of  dressing  and  tending  infants,  —  to  swad- 
dling-bands and  skirts,  to  constant  holding  in  the  arms,  or  con- 
fining in  baskets,  pouches,  and  cribs.  No  doubt  it  has  been  of 
inestimable  value  to  the  race  that  infants  have  been  thus  protected 
from  exposure  and  accident;  but  it  may  have  involved  a  minor 
disadvantage  in  breaking  up  a  normal  creeping  period.  Mrs. 
Wood's  record  and  mine  both  note  early  attempts  at  creeping, 
abandoned  in  favor  of  rolling,  a  movement  much  more  practicable 
in  skirts.  It  would  be  worth  while  to  observe  whether  nude 
babies,  left  free  in  warm  climates  to  tumble  about  on  the  ground, 
creep  earlier  It  is,  however,  possible  to  doubt  whether  four- 
footed  locomotion  was  ever  much  developed  in  the  direct  line  of 
human  ancestry  (see  below,  p.  416). 

4.  The  early  development  of  climbing  —  in  advance  of  walking 
—  and  also  the  strength  of  the  climbing  instinct.  Mrs.  Beatty's 
record  corroborates  mine  strongly  in  this  (p.  421).  Mrs.  Sharp 
mentions  climbing  before  standing  and  walking  alone  (p.  423); 
and  so  also  does  Mrs.  Wood,  if  we  are  to  understand  the  "  creeping 
upstairs"  in  the  tenth  month  (p.  423)  as  properly  climbing.  Mrs. 
Hall  does  not  record  this  movement  at  all,  and  Preyer  alone  places 
it  late,  in  the  third  year.  There  are  many  indications  that  Professor 
Preyer's  child  was  given  far  less  opportunity  for  spontaneous  mus- 
cular activity  than  the  American  children,  and  climbing  is  a  move- 
ment quite  dependent  upon  free  opportunity;  so  that  I  think  the 
other  records  on  the  whole  confirm  mine  as  to  its  early  importance. 
Airs.  Chapman  (whose  notes  I  have  several  times  quoted  in  the 
preceding  pages)  did  not  keep  regular  record  of  the  instinctive 
movements,  but  writes  me  that  her  three  children  all  climbed 
before  they  could  walk,  and  that  a  neighbor's  baby,  eleven  months 
old,  is  now  climbing,  though  unable  to  walk;  and  several  other 
cases  are  reported  to  me. 

When  we  observe  the  method  of  climbing,  placing  the  knee  or 
foot  on  a  higher  level  and  pulling  up  by  the  hands  ;  when  we  observe 
also  that  the  first  kneeling  and  standing  come  from  a  strikingly 
instinctive  tendency  to  pull  the  body  upward  with  the  hands,  which 
shows  itself  very  clearly  in  the  development  of  sitting  also;  and 
when  we  add  to  this  Dr.  Robinson's  well-known  demonstration  of 
the  monkey-like  clinging  power  of  infants'  hands  from  birth,  —  wc 


41  6  University  oj    California.  [Vol.  i. 

c;ui  hardly  fail  to  question  whether  standing  and  kneeling  may  not 
have  originated  in  climbing,  the  feet  or  knees  braced  on  a  lower 
branch,  while  the  hands  clung  to  a  higher  one.  I  do  not  overlook 
the  important  influence  that  the  use  of  the  upper  limbs  for  grasping 
and  striking  would  have  had  in  bringing  the  body  erect  and 
relegating  locomotion  to  the  lower  limbs :  but  in  my  observations 
striking  came  later  than  climbing  ;  and  though  developed  grasping 
came  much  earlier  than  developed  climbing,  the  rudiments  of  both, 
reflex  clasping  and  strong  clinging,  are  alike  present  at  birth. 

During  an  arboreal  period  which  developed  standing  and  kneel- 
ing, and  perhaps  sitting  also,  progression  on  the  ground  would  have 
been  on  all  fours.  But  as  I  have  hinted  above,  we  are  not  obliged 
to  suppose  that  this  sort  of  locomotion  ever  reached  any  high  devel- 
opment. If  we  follow  those  zoologists  who  regard  the  edentates 
(including  the  arboreal  sloths)  as  representatives  of  the  earliest 
generalized  type  of  placental  mammals,  and  the  lemurs  as  repre- 
sentatives of  the  earliest  primates,  we  trace  human  ancestry  through 
types  which  were  never  free  runners  on  all  fours,  and  which  may 
have  led  an  arboreal  life  during  the  whole  period,  merely  crawling 
slowly  on  the  ground.1  The  still  highly  general  type  of  the  human 
hand  is  against  the  supposition  that  it  was  ever  to  any  great  extent 
used  as  a  foot.  Here  we  have,  perhaps,  a  better  reason  for  the  late 
and  variable  development  of  creeping  than  the  one  suggested 
above  (p.  415). 

1  <  '.1  ling  even  back  of  the  edentates,  we  have  the  marsupials,  with  their 
Strongly  arboreal  tendency.  The  indications  seem  to  point  to  a  life  more 
arboreal  than  terrestrial  in  our  direct  line  of  ancestry  almost  or  quite  back  to 
the  first  divergence  of  mammalian  from  amphibian  types.  It  is  worth  observ- 
ing that  any  arboreal  animal  too  large  to  keep  its  young  in  holes  in  trees,  like 
squirrels,  must  necessarily  give  them  greater  care  than  a  terrestrial  animal, 
which  can  leave  them  on  the  ground:  they  must  be  constantly  held  in  pouch  or 
arms,  and  carried  about.  Here  would  be  a  strong  agency  in  bringing  about 
th.it  care  of  helpless  infancy  which,  according  to  Prof.  Fiske,  is  the  origin  of 
our  humanity.  Mrs.  Chapman  writes  of  one  of  her  boys  that  he  "climbed 
persistently  and  in  must  dangerous  places,  preferably  up  the  waste  pipe  at  the 
side  of  the  house,  and  up  perpendicular  scantlings,  until  about  seven  or  eight 
years  old,  when  he  suddenly  stopped,  and  has  never  showed  any  particular  love 
of  climbing  since"  This  passionate  impulse  to  climb,  declining  at  six  or  seven 
years  old,  is,  I  think,  not  uncommon.  It  is  evident  that  it  falls  in  well  with  the 
theory  of  a  long  arboreal  life,  ending  somewhere  about  the  early  human  period. 


shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  417 

5.  The  remarkable  joy,  pride,  and  physical  exhilaration  attend- 
ing the  acquirement  of  some  of  these  movements.  This  appears 
more  or  less  in  all  records,  though  in  several  cases  the  pleasure  was 
marred  by  timidity,  which  my  niece  never  experienced  to  a  painful 
extent. 

6.  The  similarity,  on  the  whole,  in  the  manner  of  develop- 
ment of  the  movements  in  different  children.  On  casual  compari- 
son the  variations  are  most  striking;  but  on  close  examination  of 
the  records  the  only  important  difference  seems  to  be  that  a  move- 
ment is  sometimes  held  back  a  long  time,  either  by  lack  of  oppor- 
tunity or  lack  of  confidence,  and  is  then  executed  suddenly, — a  well- 
known  occurrence  in  case  of  artificially  delayed  instincts,  as  when 
young  birds  are  prevented  from  flying  till  beyond  the  usual  time. 
For  the  rest,  certain  details  of  the  process  of  development  recur  in 
the  different  records  to  a  noticeable  extent:  as  the  wriggling  or  pull- 
ing forward  before  true  creeping,  and  the  involuntary  backward  creep- 
ing before  the  forward  movement  is  mastered  (p.  414);  the  pulling  to 
the  feet,  first  by  some  low  object  so  that  the  body  is  still  partly 
propped  by  the  arms,  then  by  a  higher  (p.  342,  and  also  Mrs. 
Beatty's  note,  p.  420),  and  later  yet  the  rising  from  all  fours.  The 
postponement  of  standing  alone  till  after  walking,  walking  till  after 
running,  in  the  case  of  Preyer's  boy,  seem  to  be  distortions  of  the 
usual  order,  due  to  deferring  of  the  instinct,  through  timidity;  and 
this  timidity  Preyer  thought  actually  increased,  to  the  delay  of  the 
development,  through  efforts  to  hasten  walking  and  standing;  and 
Mrs.  Beatty  had  a  similar  impression  with  regard  to  her  boy  (p.  421 ). 


Some  pedagogical  conclusions  are  clearly  indicated: — 
1 .  It  seems  evident  that  the  infant  should  have  by  the  second 
quarter-year  the  utmost  freedom  of  limbs  and  opportunity  of  move- 
ment on  the  floor  that  is  possible  without  exposure  to  cold  or  acci- 
dent. Any  such  exposure,  however,  is  more  serious  than  the 
restriction  of  muscular  development:  so  that  to  avoid  both  evils 
may  demand  vigilant  care.  If  the  mother  is  so  circumstanced 
that  she  can  give  this  watchfulness,  she  should  not  grudge  the 
trouble:  it  requires  a  kind  of  devotion  not  often  to  be  expected  from 
a  servant.    A  year-old  baby,  for  instance,  may  be  allowed  extensive 


4-1  &  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

climbing,  to  his  physical  and  mental  advantage,  if  some  one  is  so 
close  at  hand,  and  so  unfailing  in  attention,  that  the  little  one  can- 
not slip  without  finding  himself  instantly  safe  in  arms. 

2.  While  there  is  no  harm  in  some  little  encouragement  where 
it  seems  needed,  any  more  than  in  the  birds'  teaching  their  babies 
to  fly,  it  need  hardly  be  said  nowadays  that  none  of  these  move- 
ments should  be  urged  and  hastened,  and  especially  that  the  baby 
should  not  be  allowed  to  bear  his  own  weight,  in  sitting,  standing,  or 
walking,  till  he  is  unmistakably  able  to  ;  nor  is  it  desirable  to  urge 
a  feat  of  balancing  upon  a  timid  child,  even  when  he  is  plainly 
capable  of  it,  lest  he  get  fixed  associations  of  fear  with  it,  and  be 
actually  held  back  in  progress.  For  the  scientific  observer's  pur- 
pose, of  course,  the  more  absolutely  spontaneous  the  development 
the  better  always.  But  where  a  child  has  become  discouraged,  or 
lias  been  held  back  a  long  time  by  timidity,  a  little  cautious  coax- 
ing past  the  sticking-point  may  be  the  wisest  thing  pedagogically. 
Nothing  can  take  the  place  of  motherly  common  sense  in  such  a 
matter. 

3.  From  the  time  the  child  has  fairly  mastered  the  race  move- 
ments, the  mother  should  be  alert  to  notice  any  special  clumsiness 
of  gait  or  bearing,  and  to  determine,  by  comparison  with  other 
children,  whether  it  is  a  mere  natural  phase  of  growth,  or  the 
beginning  of  a  fixed  individual  trick.  It  will  scarcely  define  itself 
as  the  latter  within  the  first  three  years,  but  it  will  often  do  so  in 
the  next  two  or  three  ;  and  whenever  it  does  (I  may  here  go  beyond 
the  limit  of  this  record  to  say),  if  there  is  available  a  competent 
authority  in  such  matters,  able  to  determine  the  exact  fault  of 
muscular  development  that  causes  the  trick,  and  to  prescribe  plays 
and  movements  that  will  gently  correct  it,  the  mother  should 
consult  him.  T.ie  freest  activity  of  the  healthiest  child  is  not  a 
guarantee  of  symmetry.  In  the  first  difficulties  of  balancing, 
babies  may  throw  themselves  into  bad  attitudes  that  become  fixed 
habits.  Or  they  inherit  feeble  development  of  this  or  that  muscle, 
which  is  therefore  neglected  in  use,  and  becomes  still  feebler ;  and 
where  it  fails  in  its  due  tension,  the  opposite  muscle  over-pulls  the 
bone,  and  the  distortion  in  time  becomes  fixed  in  the  bony  structure. 
Thus  very  little  children  may  be  caught  habitually  standing  on  one 
foot,  resting  on  the  heels  with  abdominal  muscles  relaxed,  dropping 


Shinn. 


The  Development  of  a   Child. 


419 


the  shoulders  forward,  etc.,  —  tricks  that  are  not  only  ungraceful, 
but  likely  to  produce  spinal  curvatures,  narrowed  chests,  and  other 
serious  physical  ills.  There  is  little  use  in  the  mother's  trying  to 
correct  such  faults,  for  all  she  can  do,  even  with  older  children,  is 
constantly  to  warn  them  to  correct  their  positions ;  the  weak 
muscle  can  be  pulled  to  its  place  for  the  moment  by  an  effort,  but 
the  instant  attention  is  withdrawn,  the  muscle  relaxes,  and  nothing 
is  gained.  Yet  the  faults,  even  where  they  are  strongly  hereditary, 
are  corrected  with  singular  ease  in  these  early  years  by  special 
exercise,  as  I  know  from  the  case  of  my  niece,  who  at  seven  years 
old  showed  remarkable  improvement  in  her  whole  carriage,  as  the 
result  of  exercises,  within  a  month.  Symmetrical  muscular  devel- 
opment, however,  is  not  ordinarily  included  in  the  range  of  a 
physician's  studies  ;  nor  do  many  physicians  care  to  occupy  them- 
selves with  a  child's  turned-in  toes  or  slovenly  way  of  sitting.  Until 
competent  gymnasium  teachers,  therefore,  are  far  more  numerous 
in  the  community,  it  will  often  be  impossible  for  the  mother  to  get 
proper  advice  about  matters  of  gait  and  bearing.  In  that  case,  it 
will  be  better  for  her  to  trust  to  the  utmost  general  freedom  and 
activity  of  the  body  than  to  take  risks  from  ignorantly  prescribed 
special  treatment. 

Table  9.     Instincts  Connected  with  Food-Taking. 


EARLIEST    DATE  OF 

LATER    DATES   OK  APPEAR- 

MOVEMENT. 

OBSERVER. 

APPEARANCE. 

ANCE. 

Prever  and  other 

At  birth 

[I  do  not  know  of  any  re- 
corded    case   in     which 

this   movement  did   not 

appear  on  the  first  op- 

portunity,     usually     1st 

day ;    cases    are    indefi- 

nitely reported  in  which 

it  was  at  first  very  imper- 

fect.] 

Thrusting 

out  nipple... 
Smacking 

Biting  

10th  to  20th  months.. 

Spitting  out. 

Shinn 

Of  the  other  instinctive  movements,  described  on  pp.  392-5, 
there  is  not  enough  record  made  by  other  observers  to  be  worth 
comparative  tabulating.  Mii.icent  Washburn  Shinn. 


OTHER  RECORDS  OF  THE  INSTINCTIVE  MOVEMENTS. 

From  Mrs.  Beattv's  MS.  Record  (see  p.  22). 

In  the  eighth  month  the  baby  was  trying  to  creep,  rising  on  hands  and  toes, 
but  in  his  first  efforts  he  pushed  himself  involuntarily  backward.  This  always 
made  him  angry,  as  he  kept  getting  farther  and  farther  away  from  the  object 
he  was  trying  to  reach.  This  continued  till  late  in  the  eighth  or  early  in  the  ninth 
month.  During  the  ninth  month,  he  was  in  camp,  and  had  no  opportunity  to 
creep  except  on  the  bed;  but  I  saw  him  one  day  creeping  forward  across  the 
bed;  and  after  this  he  could  steer  in  the  direction  he  wished  to  go.  After 
returning  home  he  crept  quite  rapidly,  turning  out  his  toes.  In  the  eleventh 
month  he  sometimes  crept  on  hands  and  knees,  and  sometimes  on  hands  and 
toes. 

By  this  time  (eleventh  month)  he  was  pulling  himself  to  his  feet,  but  only  by 
rather  low  objects,  as  a  cracker-box  [so  that  his  weight  was  partly  propped  by 
his  hands,  as  in  the  case  of  my  niece  at  the  same  stage:  see  p.  342.  M.  IV.  S.I; 
but  by  the  middle  of  the  month  he  would  get  to  his  feet  by  almost  any 
object,  his  mother's  skirt,  e.  g.  He  could  now  stand  by  a  chair,  and  keep- 
ing hold  with  one  hand  could  lean  over  and  take  things  from  the  floor.  He 
rarelv  lost  balance,  and  daily  gained  more  ease  and  confidence  in  standing. 
In  the  forty-seventh  week,  however,  as  he  stood  by  a  light  kitchen  chair,  which 
moved  a  little  over  the  uncarpeted  floor  as  he  pushed  against  it,  he  became 
alarmed,  drew  back,  stood  alone  for  a  second,  then  sat  down  suddenly,  crying 
with  fright. 

About  this  same  time,  the  middle  of  the  eleventh  month  (forty-sixth  week), 
he  made  an  important  advance  toward  walking.  He  was  standing  by  his 
mother's  knee,  and  desired  to  investigate  the  contents  of  a  basket  on  a  chair 
nearby.  Reaching  out  with  one  hand,  he  steadied  himself  by  the  chair;  then 
letting  go  his  mother's  dress  with  the  other  hand,  he  tried  to  find  a  hold  for  it 
on  the  chair.  He  had  some  difficulty  in  doing  so,  bent  over  to  take  hold  of  the 
rung,  and  finding  that  would  compel  him  to  stoop,  at  last  placed  both  hands  on 
the  seat  of  the  chair,  and  stepped  over  to  it.  In  the  forty-eighth  week,  as  he 
stood  by  the  lounge,  he  reached  out  his  hand  and  caught  his  mother  by  the 
dress,  then  by  the  hand,  and  by  pulling  managed  to  step  over  to  her.  In  the 
next  few  days  he  became  able,  by  taking  my  hand,  and  pressing  his  other  hand 
against  a  wall,  to  step  along  slowly;  and  he  would  edge  all  about  large,  heavy 
chairs,  holding  to  them.  The  last  day  of  the  month  he  was  shown  how  to 
push  a  light  chair  before  him  over  a  matting-covered  floor  :  he  was  highly 
delighted  with  this  mode  of  locomotion,  and  moved  rapidly  about  the  room 
with  the  chair,  although  less  than  a  fortnight  earlier  he  had  been  so  frightened 
at  the  slight  moving  of  a  chair  under  his  hands. 

In  the  latter  part  of  this  same  active  month,  the  eleventh,  he  began  to  let 
(420) 


shinn.i  The  Development  of  a   Child.  42  I 

himself  cautiously  down  by  his  arms  from  the  lounge,  —  at  first  not  unless 
some  one  was  close  beside  him;  but  in  the  forty-eighth  week  he  repeatedly  did 
it  quite  alone.  I  was  told  in  this  week  that  he  had  climbed  up  on  the  lounge 
alone,  but  I  thought  it  very  doubtful. 


During  the  next  three  months  creeping  continued  a  constant  habit.  One 
or  two  indispositions,  and  still  more,  lack  of  confidence,  delayed  the  baby's 
progress  toward  standing  and  walking  alone,  and  in  the  latter  part  of  the  four- 
teenth month  he  still  stood  alone  only  when  he  became  interested  in  something 
and  forgot  to  hold  on  ;  at  such  times  he  now  and  then  fell  over.  In  the  sixtieth 
week  he  once  took  a  squatting  position,  preparing  to  rise  to  his  feet;  but  only 
because  he  expected  me  to  take  his  hands  and  help  him;  and  when  I  did  not 
do  so,  he  dropped  down  on  hands  and  knees. 

He  gained  gradually  in  steadiness  in  walking  by  a  support,  and  seemed 
able  to  walk  alone,  but  made  no  effort  to  do  so.  By  the  end  of  the  thirteenth 
month  he  could  walk  pretty  well  by  taking  hold  of  one  of  my  fingers,  but  he 
preferred  to  cling  to  my  dress  at  the  same  time,  with  the  other  hand.  He  now 
usually  walked  quite  fast.  By  the  end  of  the  fourteenth  month,  he  needed 
but  the  slightest  hold  on  my  finger  to  walk  easily,  he  could  push  a  chair 
all  around  the  dining-table,  and  if  it  encountered  any  obstacle  could  turn 
it  without  help;  could  walk  along  by  merely  putting  his  hand  against  the  wall: 
but  any  effort  to  have  him  walk  (mite  alone  caused  such  excitement  and 
timidity  that  we  did  not  urge  him.  At  last,  in  the  sixtieth  week,  he  took  a  step 
alone  without  knowing  it,  when  I  had  released  my  hold  on  his  arm.  A  few 
days  later  he  took  three  steps  to  reach  me,  and  the  same  day  his  father  vouched 
for  four  steps,  in  my  absence. 

His  progress  in  climbing  during  this  period  was  more  vigorous.  In  the 
latter  part  of  the  thirteenth  month  he  had  a  perfect  fever  for  climbing.  He 
would  mount  to  a  stool  by  means  of  a  chair  close  by;  would  climb  over  the 
rockers  of  the  rocking-chairs;  try  to  climb  into  boxes;  scramble  up  on  the 
lower  shelf  of  a  stand  on  one  side,  and  off  it  on  the  other;  etc. 


In  the  fifteenth  month,  the  child  for  a  time  seemed  to  have  gained  no  con- 
fidence from  his  successful  steps  alone  in  the  fourteenth;  so  that  in  the  sixty- 
fourth  week  his  father  and  I  began  to  coax  him  to  walk  from  one  to  the  other; 
and  by  gradually  increasing  the  distance  lured  him  thus  to  walk  across  the 
room  alone  several  times.  After  this  if  I  took  his  hand  off  a  chair,  he  would 
walk  alone,  but  would  not  voluntarily  quit  his  support  till  near  the  end  of  the 
week  (the  sixty-fourth).  On  the  same  day  on  which  he  first  took  his  hand  thus 
off  a  chair  to  walk  alone,  he  started  to  walk  from  his  grandmother  to  me,  but 
when  he  had  gone  half  way,  and  I  held  out  my  arms  to  receive  him,  he  sud- 
denly whirled  about  and  walked  back  to  his  grandmother,  evidently  pleased 
that  he  had  played  a  joke  on  me.  He  repeated  this  joke  several  times  in  the 
next  few  days.  The  day  after,  he  left  his  uncle's  knee  to  walk  to  his  box  of 
playthings  across  the  room,  and  get  something  he  wished;  and  after  this  he 
frequently  walked  alone,  though  he  would  still   become  timorous,  and  drop 


42  2  University  of  California.  [Vol.  i. 

down  and  creep.  It  did  not  trouble  him  to  step  over  door-sills,  though  up  to 
the  end  of  the  month  (fifteenth)  he  never  tried  them  unless  some  one  was  close 
by  to  catch  him  in  case  of  accident.  He  walked  with  his  arms  raised  and 
hands  stretched  out  toward  the  object  he  was  aiming  at,  and  often  balanced 
himself  before  starting  out. 

Early  in  the  sixteenth  month  (sixty-sixth  week),  he  was  walking  freely  all 
over  the  house,  and  with  much  less  difficulty  of  balancing,  though  he  still 
raised  his  arms  somewhat.  He  often  walked  easily  over  door-sills,  though 
sometimes  they  seemed  to  trouble  him.  He  still  dropped  down  to  creep  often, 
losing  confidence.  On  the  459th  day,  in  walking  from  one  object  to  another. 
he  would  run  the  last  few  steps,  thinking  it  great  fun.  He  improved  daily  in 
walking,  and  early  in  the  sixty-seventh  week  had  abandoned  the  practice  of 
holding  his  hands  up  to  balance;  I  even  saw  him  walking  with  an  object  in  each 
hand,  and  another  in  his  teeth.  He  had  comparatively  few  falls,  and  was  less 
disposed  to  drop  down  and  creep.  By  the  middle  of  the  month,  if  he  lost  his 
balance  he  would  pick  himself  up  and  goon.  In  the  sixty-eighth  and  sixty- 
ninth  weeks,  he  was  quite  ill,  and  became  so  weak  that  he  staggered  when  h 
walked;  but  as  soon  as  he  was  well,  returned  to  his  walking  energetically,  and 
though  not  very  firm  on  his  legs,  would  not  creep  at  all. 

Up  to  the  sixty-sixth  week  he  had  still  crept  a  great  deal,  especially  when  he 
felt  uncertain  on  his  feet,  or  happened  to  sit  down  on  the  floor,  or  was  not 
near  anything  by  which  he  could  pull  himself  up.  He  was  in  these  last  weeks 
of  creeping  fond  of  going  about  on  hands  and  feet,  and  could  travel  quite  fast. 
But  by  the  sixty-seventh  week  creeping  had  visibly  declined,  and  in  the  sixty- 
eighth  it  was  quite  rare;  by  the  last  week  of  the  month,  it  seemed  entirely 
abandoned. 

Standing  alone  made  no  progress  till  the  child  had  acquired  some  con- 
fidence in  walking ;  but  by  the  beginning  of  the  sixteenth  month  he  stood 
alone  boldly  in  the  middle  of  the  floor,  and  stooped  over  and  picked  things 
up.  In  the  next  week  (sixty-sixth),  he  lifted  himself  up  to  his  feet  from  all- 
fours:  and  by  the  middle  of  the  month  could  get  up  this  way  quite  readily;  if 
he  stepped  on  his  dress,  he  would  work  his  feet  about  till  he  got  them  free, 
then  stand  up. 

The  mania  for  climbing  is  again  recorded  in  the  sixteenth  month,  the  stairs 
now  being  the  special  object  of  desire.  In  the  twentieth  month  the  child  first 
succeeded  in  mounting  into  a  large  chair,  and  displayed  the  feat  with  charming 
pride  when  his  father  came  home. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  nineteenth  month  walking  was  abandoned  for  a 
trotting  gait. 

From  Mrs.  Sharp's  MS.  Record  (see  p.  21). 

ELDEST    BOY. 

At  just  six  months  old  the  baby  worked  himself  forward,  trying  to  reach 
something  on  the  floor :  he  was  lying  face  down,  and  stretched  his  arms 
forward,  bent  at  the  elbows,  and  resting  on  his  fore-arms,  drew  his  body 
forward.     He  began  to  creep  in  the  latter  part  of  the  seventh  month. 


Shinn.]  The  Development  of  a   Child.  423 

The  first  week  of  the  twelfth  month  he  walked  alone,  a  distance  of  three 
or  four  feet  between  two  chairs. 

YOUNGEST   BOY. 

In  the  latter  part  of  the  seventh  month,  when  laid  on  his  back,  the  baby 
turned  over  on  his  face  and  turned  back  again  with  ease,  without  bumping  his. 
head,  —  the  first  time  I  had  ever  seen  him  do  it. 

By  the  tenth  month  he  crept  rapidly.  In  the  first  week  of  this  month  he 
would  pull  himself  up  to  his  knees,  and  make  violent  efforts  to  get  on  his  feet. 
He  could  do  it  with  slight  help,  but  could  not  bear  his  weight  on  them. 

In  the  first  week  of  the  eleventh  month  he  still  needed  a  little  help  in 
getting  to  his  feet.  Yet  in  this  same  week  he  could  take  a  few  steps  pushing  a 
chair,  and  could  edge  along  the  length  of  a  sofa. 

At  a  year  old  he  walked  by  chairs  and  walls  and  other  slight  support. 
When  his  four-year-old  brother  called  him  to  play  "  choo-choo,"  he  would  pull 
himself  to  his  feet  by  the  back  of  his  brother's  dress,  and  trot  slowly  round 
the  room  after  him.  At  the  same  age,  he  climbed  into  a  double  rocking-horse, 
and  stood  erect,  holding  the  edge  of  a  blackboard  with  one  hand,  while  he 
marked  on  the  board  with  a  piece  of  chalk  in  the  other. 


From  MS.  Records  of  Mrs.  Edith  Elmer  Wood,  B.  L.,  Smith  College. 

ELDER    BOY. 

At  four  months  old,  if  supported  by  a  hand  under  the  body,  the  baby  would 
prance  the  length  of  a  bed  on  his  hands  and  feet.  In  the  sixth  month  he 
could  propel  himself  for  short  distances  in  measure-worm  fashion.  He  had 
already,  however,  in  the  fifth  month,  become  able  to  roll  over  without  help, 
and  in  the  seventh  month  began  to  occupy  himself  with  this  movement,  instead 
of  learning  to  creep.  Meantime,  by  the  middle  of  the  sixth  month,  he  had 
become  able  to  sit  alone  on  a  bed,  and  by  the  end  of  the  month  on  the  floor. 

It  was  not  until  the  ninth  month  that  he  rather  suddenly  began  to  creep, 
and  within  a  week  after  the  first  step  on  hands  and  knees  was  creeping 
across  the  room  without  stopping.  In  the  tenth  month  he  would  creep  up- 
stairs. In  the  same  month  he  could  stand,  holding  by  a  chair,  and  in  the 
eleventh  he  stood  alone.  In  the  eleventh,  too,  he  could  walk  a  few  steps, 
holding  some  one's  hand,  and  could  travel  across  the  room,  pushing  an  inverted 
waste-basket  before  him.  In  the  twelfth  he  could  take  six  or  eight  steps  alone, 
and  walked  quite  well  with  the  aid  of  some  one's  hand. 

In  the  thirteenth  month  he  could  run  alone,  and  in  the  fourteenth  could 
walk  up  and  down  stairs  with  the  aid  of  a  hand.  By  the  sixteenth  month  notes 
implying  perfectly  free  and  confident  roaming  about  occur,  —  chasing  the  cat, 
for  instance;  and  in  the  twentieth  he  was  a  great  little  gad-about,  always 
seizing  you  by  the  hand  and  commanding  you  to  '"m  'on!"  In  the  nineteenth 
month  he  was  very  active  in  climbing  on  chairs,  tables,  etc.,  and  tried  to  climb 
trees:  in  the  twentieth  and  twenty-first  also  varied  and  skillful  climbing  is  noted. 


424  University  of  California.  [Vol.  1 

YOUNGER    BOY. 

The  baby  made  efforts  to  lift  his  head  on  the  first  day,  raising  it  clear  from 
the  pillow. 

Early  in  the  third  month,  when  propped  at  an  angle  of  45°,  he  would 
lift  himself  to  a  sitting  position,  tipping  over  at  once;  and  when  flat  on  his 
back,  he  would  lift  his  head  and  shoulders  clear.  He  had  a  great  desire  to  sit 
up.  At  three  months  old  he  could  sit  alone  on  a  bed,  but  his  balance  was  still 
insecure  at  five  months  old.     At  six  months  he  could  sit  alone  on  the  floor. 

In  the  eighth  month  he  tried  hard  to  creep :  he  would  place  himself  on 
hands  and  knees  correctly,  and  move  his  hands,  but  could  not  move  his  legs 
after  them,  and  would  soon,  therefore,  be  flat  on  his  stomach,  after  which  he 
would  wriggle  along.  Toward  the  end  of  the  month  he  resorted  to  rolling,  and 
though  he  could  not  at  first  steer  well,  he  could  move  about  the  floor  quite 
rapidly,  and  in  the  ninth  month  learned  to  steer  himself  well.  In  the  tenth 
month  he  would  pull  himself  over  the  floor  by  his  arms  and  by  pushing  with 
his  feet. 

It  was  not  until  the  thirteenth  month  that  he  at  last  began  to  creep  properly. 
He  could  by  this  time  take  a  few  steps,  pushing  a  chair  in  front  of  him  ;  and 
by  the  end  of  the  month  he  could  walk  about  pretty  well,  holding  to  a  finger. 


Date  Due 

M/WH*-r 

■•     •      ■£/     -J 

;^.* 

bee.  22.  '8/ 

t    && 

i    1 

Library  Bureau  Cat.  no.  1 137 


qLB1131  -S5 


3  5002  02004  1500 


Shinn,  Milicent  Washburn 

Notes  on  the  development  of  a  child  . 


PfcVCHOUJutf  Ud<<A^ 


1131 
S5 


\Vo^^S\^ 


Ill 


ill 


II 


II 


.,